Skip to main content

Full text of "The Trend Of Our Civilization Vol XIX"

See other formats


TIGHT BINDING BOOK 



a: 
LU < 



^ CD 



OU164105>m 



OSMANIA UNIVERSITV LIBRARY 

Call No. 3 o (, / -ft^, 5" I T Accession No. &.-( O 



Author 



This book should be returned on or before the date last marked below. 



PAPERS AND PROCEEDINGS 

NINETEENTH ANNUAL MEETING 

AMERICAN 
SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

HELD AT CHICAGO 
DECEMBER 29-31, 1924 



VOLUME XIX 



THE TREND OF OUR CIVILIZATION 



PUBLISHED FOR THE 
AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

BY 

THE UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO PRESS 
CHICAGO, ILLINOIS 



COPYRIGHT 1925 BY 
THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 



All Rights Reserved 



Published October 1925 



Composed and Printed By 

The University of Chicago Press 

Chicago, Illinois, U.S.A. 



TABLE OF CONTENTS 

PAGE 

INTOLERANCE. Charles A . Ellwood i 

PYSCHOLOGY AND CULTURE. Alexander Goldenweiser 15 

CULTURE AND CULTURAL TRENDS. Robert E. Park 24 

THE SUBJECTIVE ASPECT OF CULTURE. Ellsworth Paris 37 

A BIOLOGICAL VIEW OF RACE MIXTURE. L. C. Dunn 47 

DISCUSSION. F. H. Ilankins 56 

THE HYBRID AS A SOCIOLOGICAL TYPE. E. B. Renter 59 

AN ANTHROPOLOGICAL VIEW or RACE MIXTURE. Ralph Linton ... 69 
DISCUSSION. Kimball Young 77 

INTERNATIONAL ASPECTS OF THE RUSSIAN REVOLUTION. Jerome Davis . 80 

SOCIOLOGICAL FACTORS IN THE INTERPRETATION OF INTERNATIONAL RE- 
LATIONS: WITH SPECIFIC ILLUSTRATIONS FROM SOUTHEASTERN 
EUROPE AND THE NEAR EAST. Earle Edward Eubank ... 88 

POPULATION DENSITIES AND THE IMMIGRATION POLICY OF THE UNITED 

STATES. Niles Carpenter 97 

THE NEED FOR IMPROVED CHILD-WELFARE STATISTICS. Robert M. 

Woodbury 109 

THE DEVELOPMENT OF AMERICAN VITAL STATISTICS. Walter F. Willcox 123 

SECTION ON RURAL SOCIOLOGY: SIGNIFICANT FACTORS IN RURAL POPULA- 
TION AFFECTING OUR CIVILIZATION 

FARM POPULATIONS. John M. Gillette 135 

VILLAGE POPULATIONS. C. Luther Fry 143 

RURAL DEMOGRAPHY. Warren S. Thompson 150 

THE SHIFTING BASES OF RURAL SOCIOLOGY. William L. Bailey . . 161 

SECTION ON SOCIAL RESEARCH 

In charge of W. F. Ogburn 

CONTRIBUTIONS FOR CHARITY, RELIGION, AND EDUCATION OF THE 
INCOME TAXPAYERS OF DANE COUNTY, WISCONSIN, 1922. John 
L. Gillin 165 

THE CHILDREN OF THE POOR. Hornell Hart 168 



vi TABLE OF CONTENTS 

PAGE 

RELATIVE RATE OF CHANGE IN CUSTOMS AND BELIEFS OF MODERN 

JEWS. Jessie Ravitch 171 

PERSONALITY AND LIFE-HISTORY DOCUMENTS. E. T. Krueger . . 176 
HISTORY TEXTBOOKS AND INTERNATIONAL DIFFERENCES. Donald 

R. Taft 180 

SOME RESEARCHES IN RURAL GROUP ANALYSIS. /. H. Kolb . . . 183 

COMMODITY DISTRIBUTION IN RURAL COMMUNITIES. C. R. Hofer . 186 
AN ESTIMATE OF RURAL MIGRATION AND OTHER SOURCES OF 

URBAN INCREASE. John M. Gillette 189 

ASPECTS AND TENDENCIES OF THE RACE PROBLEM, Monroe N. Work 191 
DENSITY OF POPULATION AND THE STANDARD OF LIVING IN NORTH 

CHINA. C. G. Dittmer 196 

A DEPENDENCY INDEX FOR MINNEAPOLIS. F. Stuart Chapin . . 200 

REPORTS OF COMMITTEES OF THE SOCIETY 

COMMITTEE ON SOCIAL RESEARCH. William F. Ogburn .... 203 

COMMITTEE ON SOCIAL ABSTRACTS. F. Stuart Chapin 211 

COMMITTEE ON AN ENCYCLOPEDIA OF THE SOCIAL SCIENCES. H. B. 

Woolston 211 

COMMITTEE ON THE TEACHING OF SOCIAL STUDIES IN THE SCHOOLS. 

Hornell Ear t 212 

COMMITTEE ON INTERNATIONAL RELATIONS. H. A. Miller . . . 216 
COMMITTEE ON PERSONNEL CLASSIFICATION IN THE FEDERAL GOV- 
ERNMENT. Carl Kelsey, R. R. Kern, Mollie R. Carroll . . . . 217 

REPORTS OF REPRESENTATIVES OF THE SOCIETY 

THE SOCIAL-SCIENCE RESEARCH COUNCIL. F. Stuart Chapin . . 219 
DELEGATES TO THE COUNCIL OF LEARNED SOCIETIES. William F. 

Ogburn 223 

REPORT OF THE MEETING OF THE SECTION ON EDUCATIONAL SOCIOLOGY 223 

PROGRAM OF THE NINETEENTH ANNUAL MEETING 225 

REPORT OF THE SECRETARY 229 

REPORT OF THE FINANCE COMMITTEE 230 

REPORT OF THE MANAGING EDITOR 234 

MINUTES OF THE EXECUTIVE COMMITTEE MEETING 235 

MINUTES OF THE BUSINESS MEETING FOR REPORTS OF COMMITTEES . 236 

MINUTES OF THE ANNUAL BUSINESS MEETING 237 

MEMBERSHIP LIST 239 



INTOLERANCE 



CHARLES A. ELLWOOD 



ABSTRACT 

Intolerance. Intolerance is in abundant evidence in every phase of American 
social life. The existence of a vast secret organization, numbering millions, which aims 
at the control of our social, political, and religious life is ample proof. Social science is 
far enough advanced to indicate the social results which we may expect from such in- 
tolerance. Progress is difficult if not impossible in an intolerant society. Intolerance 
breeds separation, misunderstanding, and hostility between groups. When groups have 
no opportunity to settle their disagreements by discussion, they are apt to resort to 
fighting. Intolerance therefore plays a leading part in fostering civic disorders, and espe- 
cially in fostering revolution. ''Repression is the seed of revolution." The particular 
repression which breeds revolution is the suppression of freedom in intercommunication : 
for the mechanism of intercommunication is the organ of adjustment for conscious social 
change. Present widespread intolerance threatens to bring on revolution. The remedy 
is the conversion of our people to the scientific attitude of mind. 



Some of you, at least, know that I have long stood for a larger 
measure of good will in human relations than is expressed by the 
word "tolerance." But it may well be questioned whether in the 
present condition of our world it would not be wiser to advocate 
tolerance in our social life, as a first step, before any higher form of 
social good will is aimed at. Last year a public session of our Society 
was devoted to discussing the question of whether or not intolerance 
is increasing in the United States. While no definite conclusion was 
reached, all speakers seemed to agree that intolerance was in abun- 
dant evidence in almost every phase of American social life. One 
member of our Society, who had traveled far and wide over the 
United States to investigate the growth and ramifications of the Ku 
Klux Klan, sorrowfully said, "I think we must conclude that we are 
essentially an intolerant people." 

I hope that we shall not be forced to reach any such conclusion. 
However, no student of our social life would deny that there has 
been, owing to the world- war or other causes, a great growth of many 
forms of intolerance among our people within the last two decades. 
In May, 1923, I talked with that veteran publisher, Mr. William 



2 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

Appleton, whom many of you knew and respected. Mr, Appleton, 
then seventy-eight years of age, had been in intimate contact with 
public men and public affairs in both England and the United States 
for more than half a century. I chanced to ask him whether in his 
long life he had ever known a period of greater intolerance than the 
present. He thought a moment and then replied, "No, not even 
during and directly after our Civil War." Then he instanced how 
two textbooks in American history, written by eminent historical 
scholars, had recently been excluded from the public schools of New 
York City because they taught that the American Revolution was 
a part of the general democratic movement among English-speaking 
peoples. 

Of course, the testimony of one man, no matter how wide his 
experience, amounts to little. But one meets this testimony regard- 
ing the intolerance of our time and country on every hand. A prom- 
inent public-school teacher from one of the large cities on our Pacific 
Coast has told me that the really able teachers of that city do not 
wish to be in the central office for the administration of its schools, 
because that office can propose nothing progressive in an educational 
way, especially in the way of social and political education, without 
being waited upon by representative business men protesting against 
any innovation. Apparently these business men believe that social 
and economic education of the children in the schools is fraught with 
danger. 

Similar testimony of intolerance on the part of business men 
comes from the eminent Boston merchant, Mr. Edward A. Filene. 
In a recent article 1 Mr. Filene has said: "Over and over again, in 
organizations of business men, I have seen successful men turn 
against and label as dangerous one of their fellows who was only 
reasonably progressive. I have seen such men display an utter inabil- 
ity to distinguish between sane social advance and revolutionary 
socialism." 

But the worst examples of intolerance are to be found, not in our 
business and industrial world, but in the religious world. The revival 
of religious bigotry and intolerance in American society is, indeed, 
one of the most startling and disturbing phenomena of our time. 

1 Harper's Magazine, December, 1923, 



INTOLERANCE 3 

Twenty years ago many of us believed that religious intolerance 
was rapidly disappearing. The Fundamentalist and kindred move- 
ments in the religious world, however, have shown us that we were 
mistaken. During the past year one of my graduate students investi- 
gated the teaching of the social sciences in sixty-two colleges of a 
prominent Protestant denomination in our southern states. It is 
perhaps sufficient to say that he discovered that there was little free- 
dom in these colleges, in the teaching of those sciences, when they 
were tolerated at all. Not only was the pressure of social, political, 
and economic orthodoxy in evidence, but religious orthodoxy prac- 
tically forbade the teaching of organic evolution, and hence, of loyal- 
ty to modern science. It was with difficulty that these facts were 
gathered, because members of faculties were loath to talk about con- 
ditions in their institutions, and especially about the ban of the 
church on the teaching of evolution. One teacher, who at first re- 
fused to say anything, finally wrote as follows: "All right. This is a 
graveyard. We are all evolutionists. Isn't it awful ? If any member 
of our faculty is not, I don't know it. Those whose subjects touch 

evolution are theistic evolutionists But we are not fools and 

do not have it served up to us in the classroom, except when the 
textbook expresses it." And he added despairingly: "When will this 
blind antagonism to Christian evolution die out ? I hope that it is 
decreasing, but I fear not." 

Surely this is the voice of a soul in prison, as truly as any that we 
find in the darkest ages of the world's history. Here is a man suffer- 
ing for conscience's sake as clearly as did any of the heroes of the 
Renaissance or the Reformation. I am inclined to believe from the 
facts that have come to me that there are hundreds of such in the 
colleges of this country. 

Nor is intolerance confined to denominational colleges. The evi- 
dence published by the American Association of University Profes- 
sors shows that it is all too frequent in some of our largest and lead- 
ing universities. Nor is intolerance simply manifested by those in 
positions of authority. It is sometimes shown by members of the 
faculty toward one another. It is even more, of course, to be found 
among the mass of the people who make up the constituency of the 
institution. Often what is taken to be the intolerance of authorities 



4 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

is, upon investigation, found to be but their response to intolerant 
public sentiment. This is a matter which directly concerns all of us; 
not simply in a personal sense, but even more the development of 
the sciences in which we are interested. Some careful students find 
that the slow development of the social sciences in our institutions 
of learning is due, in the main, to a popular intolerance among our 
people, which is, at bottom, hostile to the scientific investigation of 
social, political, and economic questions. 

If one wished detailed evidence for the existence of widespread 
popular intolerance at the present time, one would only have to 
study the rise and phenomenal growth of a vast secret order among 
us, said to number millions. For this organization, however lofty 
its pretentions, springs from racial, religious, and political intoler- 
ance, and in every community into which it is introduced it feeds 
intolerance. Here is an intolerant secret organization which aims at 
nothing less than the control of our political, economic, and religious 
life. It is unnecessary to say that its very existence is inconsistent 
with those professions of religious, political, and racial toleration 
upon which our government was founded. 

It is often said in defense of all these manifestations of intolerance 
that science itself is intolerant; that when truth is discovered, we 
cannot tolerate error; that science does not tolerate the belief that 
two and two make five; that the era of toleration is prescientific, and 
is past or passing. But this is surely a mistake. One has to acknowl- 
edge sorrowfully, to be sure, that often men working in the scientific 
field have shown an extremely intolerant spirit toward views which 
differed from their own conclusions. But this is not the true spirit 
of science. On the contrary, the very essence of the scientific 
spirit is its open-mindedness, and so its tolerance. The scientific 
spirit is simply the open-minded love of truth. Science exercises 
no compulsion upon anyone to accept its conclusions, except the 
compulsion of honesty and intelligence. It simply assembles the 
evidence, the facts, and invites anyone to judge for himself. If any 
other conclusion is warranted by the facts, science is willing to 
accept it. Science exercises no authority to make anyone believe 
even that two and two are four. It simply points to the experience of 
life as forbidding any other conclusion. As Professor Wolfe has ably 



INTOLERANCE 5 

shown, 1 the scientific mind is impersonal, skeptical, critical, tolerant, 
patient, and fearless in facing facts. It is unimpressed by social 
prestige or authority, or by social conventions. It is honest and dis- 
interested. The popular mind, on the other hand, is credulous, un- 
critical, impatient, intolerant, fearful of intellectual changes, con- 
ventional, and controlled by personal interest. In other words, in- 
tolerance springs largely from ignorance and from the lack of a scien- 
tific attitude toward social questions. 

But it is not my purpose to set forth the psychological causes 
of intolerance, nor do I wish even to affirm that it is increasing in the 
United States. It is rather my wish to inquire into the social effects 
of intolerance. Were our forefathers right in believing that political, 
economic, religious, and even racial toleration is necessary in a 
democratic society ? Or were they simply under the spell of that 
worship of the individual, political, and religious liberty which char- 
acterized the eighteenth and the nineteenth centuries ? If we tolerate 
intolerance, in other words, in any of the important phases of our 
social life, what effects may we expect ? Social science is surely far 
enough advanced to answer clearly this question. 

I would like to point out at the beginning that the essence of 
social intolerance is to be found in the suppression of the free expres- 
sion of opinions upon social, political, and economic questions. When 
people dare no longer communicate thair opinions, when they fear 
to state their grievances, when they are not at liberty to educate 
one another by free and open discussion, then indeed we have the 
essence of intolerance; for freedom of thinking, of belief, and of com- 
munication is the very essence of personal freedom. Moreover, the 
studies of sociologists and social psychologists have conclusively 
demonstrated that the mechanism of intercommunication is the 
normal means by which a group readjusts its behavior. Through 
intercommunication it is possible for a stimulus which affects only 
a few members of the group to be diffused throughout the whole 
group. Therefore the mechanism of intercommunication in a group 
functions very much the same as the nervous system functions in 
the individual. It is an organ of adaptation. If its free working is 
interfered with, normal readjustment is rendered difficult, if not 

1 Conservatism, Radicalism, and Scientific Method, pp. 222 ff. 



6 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

impossible. Freedom of. intercommunication is not therefore so 
much an individual right as a necessity for a healthy group life; 
or rather, it is a precious individual right, precisely because it is a 
necessity for normal social life. 

That we may see that this is no mere analogy, let us outline in a 
few words how intercommunication works to mediate and control 
the process of readjustment in a human group. Public criticism is a 
process of discrimination of whatever is wrong or whatever is unad- 
justed in the habits of a group. In other words, public criticism 
marks the bad working of some social custom or institution. It dis- 
criminates the elements which are working badly, and these discrim- 
inations are communicated to the whole group for its judgment. 
Discussion of the situation then develops in the group. At first this 
discussion is of a critical nature, but later the discussion, if allowed 
to proceed freely, normally takes a constructive direction. In the 
discussion many ideas come into competition and are tested out. 
Wrong ideas have their weaknesses shown, new ideas are stimulated, 
the useful, as well as the detrimental, elements in the old situation 
are discriminated, and gradually constructive views get formulated 
and new policies approved. Thus we have the formation of a group 
opinion which becomes the basis for a new adjustment in group 
behavior. 

This is the mechanism of conscious social change under normal 
conditions in a human group. It has characterized all human groups 
from primitive times, and is slowly perfecting itself at the present 
time. Probably the chief argument for democracy is that it frees 
and develops this process of conscious social change through the 
development of a social consciousness and a public opinion in the 
whole group. Obviously freedom of intercommunication is funda- 
mental in this process. 

If the process of public discussion is to be effective in helping 
groups to find solutions for their problems, freedom of thought and 
freedom of speech must be preserved. Where public criticism of 
social habits and institutions is not tolerated it is evident that their 
faults cannot be brought to the attention of the group. Tolerance of 
criticism is therefore the first condition of conscious social change, or 
rational social adjustment. It is only through such tolerance that 



INTOLERANCE ^ 

there can be in a group the greatest opportunity for the co-operative 
working of intelligence in the building up of habits, institutions, and 
policies. Only thus can grievances of individuals and classes be 
brought to public attention and the richest results of experience 
brought to bear upon a given social situation. Only thus, accord- 
ingly, is there the greatest chance of a wise and rational solution of 
public problems. It is not an accident, therefore, that those civilized 
societies which have maintained the best conditions for free inter- 
communication, free public discussion, and free formation of public 
opinion have been, on the whole, most progressive, and have shown 
the most normal, uninterrupted social development. In other words, 
those societies which have been most tolerant, politically, religiously, 
industrially, have shown, so far as can be judged by rational stand- 
ards, the most normal social development. 

Let us now look upon the other side, and notice the effects of 
intolerance of public criticism and of free discussion of public ques- 
tions upon the life of a group. We shall pass over its effects upon 
individual character. Suffice to say that it is notorious that an in- 
tolerant social atmosphere produces sycophancy, hypocrisy, moral 
cowardice, and other undesirable traits of character in individuals; 
for in such an atmosphere the individual cannot remain true to his 
conscience, intellectually honest, and sincere, but in order to prosper 
has to become a mere conformist to the order which surrounds him. 
With these individual effects of social intolerance, however, we are 
not now concerned, but only with its larger social aspects. 

First of all, we have to note that there is little chance for progress 
in an intolerant group. Progress or change in such a group can only 
come through the grace of its governing class; and usually a govern- 
ing class is interested in maintaining conditions as they are. There- 
fore, a static condition of society is apt to result. But to understand 
exactly why this is so, let us look at this matter a little more closely. 
All changes in a human group, so far as we know, are initiated by vari- 
ations in the reactions of individuals. In other words, changes start 
in human groups with variations in feeling, thought, and behavior of 
individuals. Some of these variations may, of course, be harmful 
to the group, and for that matter, to the development of the social 
life of humanity. But when all innovation along a given line tends to 



8 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

be repressed, there is no way of testing out whether the variation 
is a useful one or not. The experience of mankind has shown, there- 
fore, that the variant individual should be regarded with tolerance 
by his group; for only the rational consideration of his innovations 
by the whole group can test out their value. Moreover, modern 
societies have found that unlikeness in individuals is frequently as 
valuable as likeness for purposes of division of labor, group organi- 
zation, and group action. Too great uniformity in individual char- 
acter, opinion, and behavior is, therefore, not desirable in a civilized 
society. Moreover, sociologists would agree that the limits of differ- 
ences which are socially valuable, and so should be tolerated, are 
much greater than what the popular mind supposes. For these rea- 
sons a society which does not tolerate freedom of thought and free- 
dom of expression in individuals is bound not only to become static, 
but to lessen its efficiency as a group in a number of directions. The 
surest way to promote social progress, in other words, is to keep 
social institutions plastic by encouraging within reasonable limits 
the innovating individual, by keeping open the channels of inter- 
communication and of public criticism, and by seeing that every 
new idea and policy has a fair chance to be tested out in the forum 
of public discussion. On the other hand, the surest way to stop all 
social progress and insure a static civilization is to discourage the 
innovating individual, to frown upon public criticism of established 
institutions, and to close, so far as possible, channels for the spread 
of new ideas. That the world has approximated this static condition 
in various times and places the history of the Middle Ages in Europe 
and of Asiatic civilizations abundantly attests. 

Another result of social intolerance is that it tends to divide a 
group into misunderstanding, hostile classes. Intolerance of any 
sort bars the way to that sympathetic understanding of individuals 
and classes which is the first step toward appreciation, socialization, 
and voluntary co-operation. If we want to assimilate any element 
into our group, as, for example, the foreign-born, we must maintain 
an attitude of tolerance toward them; for any intolerance shown 
them is almost certain to create in them attitudes which will hinder 
their assimilation. Moreover, intolerance keeps individuals and 
classes apart and breeds misunderstanding between them. They 



INTOLERANCE g 

have no opportunity to talk over their differences, and when men 
cannot settle their differences by discussion, they are apt to resort to 
fighting. Intolerance, in other words, tends to breed war within the 
group and, ultimately, group disruption. I shall return to this point 
again when I consider the cause of the great civil disorders of our 
time. 

Another result of an intolerant social atmosphere is the effect 
which it has upon those who are in charge of the machinery of social 
control of the group, that is, upon officials in church, in state, and 
in industry. Such officials reflect, often in an exaggerated way, the 
intolerant spirit of the group which they represent. They become 
apprehensive and frightened at the least failure of individuals to 
conform to the standards which have been set up; hence they inaugu- 
rate a policy of repression, which, sooner or later, arouses resentment 
and resistance in some part of the group. By repression I mean any 
policy which constantly thwarts the expression of natural impulses 
and tendencies on the part of individuals. If such thwarting seems 
natural and inevitable, as when caused by hard conditions of 
life, by famine, or by public calamity, it is usually endured by the 
people with patience. This may be true even under a governmental 
system, which is strongly supported by a tradition that is regarded 
as more or less sacred, especially when there is comparatively little 
popular enlightenment. But when institutional repression is con- 
ceived of as arbitrary or unnecessary, it arouses resentment and 
resistance, and in certain elements of the group the attitude of re- 
sistance develops until finally the supreme end of life becomes, for 
these elements, the doing away with the repression. It is in this way 
that societies often make enemies for themselves. This is especially 
apt to be the case if expressions against the repressing institution 
and statements of grievances are not tolerated. 

We are now prepared to see the full social effects of intolerance in 
a dynamic society, such as ours is. As Professor Wolfe has said in 
effect, 1 in a static society intolerance and a policy of repression may 
result merely in submissive conformity, but "in a dynamic state 
no such policy of suppression can succeed. In the long run it will 
produce catastrophic revolution in the place of evolutionary readap- 

1 Conservatism, Radicalism, and Scientific Method, p. 142. 



io TEE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

tation." In other words, in a dynamic society intolerance which 
results in a policy of repression, if long continued, produces revolu- 
tion. The reasons for this are clear. A dynamic society is one neces- 
sarily in constant readjustment with its environment. The law of 
its life is change. Such changes, however, as we have seen, can take 
place only through the initiative of individuals, through free com- 
munication of stimuli and ideas throughout the group, and through 
the free formation of a new group opinion. If the expression of ideas 
on the part of the individuals in this process is repressed, the machin- 
ery of social readjustment is interfered with, and the whole group is 
apt to be thrown out of equilibrium. While the dissatisfaction at 
first may be confined to a few individuals, it is bound, sooner or later, 
to spread to the mass of the group. A policy of repression, in other 
words, in a dynamic group, destroys the plasticity of the group, and 
so destroys the basis of its security. 

This theory of the origin of social revolutions was perhaps never 
better expressed than when President Wilson said in one of his public 
addresses, "Repression is the seed of revolution." It is not too much 
to say that this pregnant phrase nearly expresses the modern psycho- 
logical and sociological view. All scientific psychological study of the 
effects of repression upon the individual has substantiated this 
theory. Nevertheless, this "repression theory of revolutions," 1 as 
we may call it, has not received widespread acceptance, probably 
because it seems to throw the burden of responsibility for causing 
revolutions upon the conservative and ruling classes. The spokes- 
men of these classes have often said, on the other hand, that revolu- 
tions are caused by the false hopes that are awakened among the 
masses by Utopian thinkers, who present impossible social ideals. 
Because of these ideals, people become discontented, and this dis- 
content with existing institutions is gradually diffused among the 
ignorant masses through the force of suggestion and imitation until 
at last these ignorant masses develop an attitude of revolt. They 
cite as an example the Russian revolution. It will be noted that 
this theory assumes that the mass of the people are irrational, and 
may be made discontented by agitators merely by suggestion and 

1 This theory was outlined by the writer in his Sociology in Its Psychological Aspects 
(1912), pp. 163-73, and in his Introduction to Social Psychology (1917), pp. 170-83. 



INTOLERANCE II 

imitation when they have no rational ground for discontent. The 
theory assumes a force to suggestion and imitation in the social life 
which critical psychology and sociology do not find that they pos- 
sess. While it is true that the mass of men have no highly developed 
rationality, yet on the other hand, men are inert creatures of habit, 
and rarely manifest discontent, especially in the extreme form of 
the attitude of revolt, without considerable cause. We have no evi- 
dence which warrants the belief that masses of men get discontented 
over vain imaginings, or can be easily stampeded by suggestions 
which are not in line with the situation in which they find them- 
selves. Men rarely undertake civil war between classes, any more 
than war between nations, without considerable incitement to con- 
flict; in other words, without serious grievances. Utopian and radical 
thinkers do not cause revolutions, but rather voice discontent which 
already exists. They may further revolutionary movements, but 
they do not cause them. Such movements are caused by the dis- 
content which naturally arises from the thwarting of human im- 
pulses and desires. In other words, the real cause or stimulus which 
provokes the revolution must be sought always in the system of 
social control. When that system is immobile, inflexible, and especi- 
ally when it becomes repressive of free expression on the part of indi- 
viduals that is, when it interferes with the free functioning of the 
process of intercommunication, of group discussion, with the forma- 
tion of group opinion, and the determination of group policies it is 
bound sooner or later to bring about dissatisfaction and revolt in the 
masses of the people. 

All this is clearly illustrated by the case of Russia. So far from 
the Russian revolution being the work of agitators and of Utopian 
idealists, the studies of President Masaryk, of Professor Ross, and of 
many other careful students have shown conclusively that it origi- 
nated in policies of repression which had continued for over a cen- 
tury. The Russian revolution was destructive and terrible just 
because the repressions which had preceded it were severe and pro- 
longed. The French Revolution also illustrates, not less clearly, 
the part which repression plays in causing social explosions. But it 
would be a mistake to think that such repression and interference 
with normal social change is always the work of a governing class, 



12 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

or of a small selfish minority. On the contrary, it may sometimes be 
the work of an intolerant majority. The history of our own country 
illustrates this. Early in the nineteenth century there was still hope 
in the United States that the slavery question might be settled 
peaceably by discussion and by rational public opinion. But after 
1830 popular sentiment in our South became intolerant of criticism 
of the institution of slavery, opposing public discussion of the institu- 
tion in any way. The result was that the institution remained 
relatively unchanged, until the Civil War a war essentially revolu- 
tionary in character swept the institution away. Intolerant public 
sentiments and beliefs may give rise, therefore, to policies of repres- 
sion, and to inflexibility in habits and institutions which may stop 
normal social development and pave the way to later social disaster. 
It is perhaps well to remember here that "class interest/' both of 
privileged and unprivileged classes, on account of the tendency of 
all groups to group egoism, is liable to give rise to intolerance and to 
attempts to suppress public criticism of class policies and actions 
whenever it can. Back of this mistaken policy, of course, stands the 
attitude of intolerance. Social experience seems to show that if those 
in power, whether they represent a minority or a majority, will seek 
to keep open the means of understanding and sympathy between 
classes; if they will keep untrammeled public criticism and discussion 
of public policies, and all the means of forming rational public opin- 
ion and of selecting authorities to carry out the same, there will be 
little danger of catastrophic revolution being resorted to in any social 
group. 

I have now given my answer to the question as to whether our 
forefathers were right in believing that political, religious, economic, 
and racial toleration is necessary in a democratic society. As one 
of our most careful thinkers on social theory has said, 1 in a demo- 
cratic society "it is probably unlikely, taking everything into con- 
sideration, that the quality of tolerance will ever be excessive." All 
social experience goes to show that a democratic society can have no 
safety without tolerance; for, as I have already said, when men can- 
not settle their differences by peaceful discussion, they are apt to 
resort to fighting. It has been the pride of English-speaking peoples, 

1 Balz, The Basis of Social Theory, p, 243. 



INTOLERANCE 13 

from the days of Magna Charta to the present, that they have learn- 
ed to settle their social and political questions by discussion rather 
than by fighting. But there seems just at present some danger that 
this great tradition of our democracy may be forgotten. We seem 
about to lose our faith in open public discussion as a means of settling 
social, political, and economic disputes. Not only in Europe, but in 
the United States also, there is evidence of a trend toward thinking 
that public questions can be settled by force or coercion. Hence, in 
spite of the fact that modern psychology unites with social science 
in demonstrating the futility and danger of this method, there has 
been a growth of popular intolerance which favors repression and 
coercion as a means of settling problems. 

What is the remedy? The one radical remedy for the spirit of 
intolerance and the dangers with which it threatens us, as Professor 
Wolfe points out, is the conversion of our people to the scientific 
attitude. Nothing short of the diffusion of the scientific attitude can 
free our people from that control by selfish personal and class inter- 
ests which renders them intolerant toward new ideas and toward 
every proposed change. The impersonal open-mindedness and in- 
tellectual honesty of the scientific spirit is absolutely necessary 
for a people who undertake to rule themselves through rational 
public opinion. If we still find evidence at times of a spirit of intoler- 
ance among those who profess the scientific attitude, it must be said 
that this is because they have acquired it only in part, and not to- 
ward every phase of life. 

It must be acknowledged, however, that tolerance helps the 
development of the scientific spirit quite as much as the scientific 
spirit develops tolerance. "What makes a Liberal," Professor Gil- 
bert Murray has said, "is liberality toward new ideas and toward 
opponents, readiness to hear reason, and anxiety not to be misled 
by prejudice, nor to fall back on mere authority or coercion. Surely 
such liberalism is a long step toward the scientific attitude. The 
alliance of liberalism and science is, therefore, not an accident. 
Science, no more than democracy, can afford to tolerate intolerance. 

As scientific men, as well as patriotic citizens, we have every 
reason to oppose intolerance, and to do all we can to promote toler- 
ance. Hardly any of us, I imagine, would deny that the supreme 



14 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

values of human life lie in intelligence, in good will toward our fellow- 
men, and in the good will of others toward us. Intolerance means 
the negation of all of these values. Tolerance, on the other hand, 
furthers their realization. We all recognize that tolerance is a means 
of developing a broader emotional life. Should we not equally 
recognize that it is indispensable for the development of a truly 
broad intellectual life, and so for the spread of that scientific attitude 
among our people which must be the hope of the future? 



PSYCHOLOGY AND CULTURE 



ALEXANDER GOLDENWEISER 
The New School for Social Research 



ABSTRACT 

Psychology and culture. All culture, in the last analysis, emanates from the in- 
dividual, but the individual psyche itself owes its content to culture. This apparent 
paradox has led some to exaggerate the significance of the individual, others, that of 
culture. Both must be taken cognizance of in any fair analysis of culture. The individ- 
ual and society. Man is the sensory factor in society, society is made possible by in- 
dividuals; but the problem of the relation of the individual to society is not a fictitious 
one. It is, in fact, a complex problem allowing of many solutions and presenting new 
fields for investigation. Social situations can be arranged in a series representing levels 
of socialization, the highest level being represented by a crowd, in the narrow sense, the 
lowest by a student engaged in the study of an abstract subject. Psychology and culture 
from the methodological standpoint. "Psychic unity" is imbedded in the original nature 
of man. From this spring cultural features of universal distribution, which can be inter- 
preted psychologically. When the features are general but not universal, both historical 
and psychological interpretations remain possible, but the separation of the two often 
presents insuperable difficulties. Local features can only be interpreted historically. 
Historical explanations do not preclude psychology. There is reality in such concepts as 
the primitive mind or the German mind which, however, rest in history, not in biology. 
The modern cultural situation is a laboratory for the study of both the individual and 
culture. The perplexities of the semi-detached individual reveal aspects of the original 
nature, the difficulties of a hard-pressed culture make possible new insight into the 
nature of culture. 

The purpose of this paper is to present in a brief formulation the 
relations between psychological facts and cultural facts, the approach 
being analytical as well as methodological. 

I. THE MOST GENERAL RELATION BETWEEN PSYCHOLOGY 
AND CULTURE 

To a superficial view the relation between the individual psyche 
and culture seems paradoxical; on the one hand, culture springs 
from the individual psyche, on the other, the individual psyche itself 
is determined by culture. 

If this proposition is expanded, the paradox disappears. If cul- 
ture could be analyzed retrospectively every element of it would be 
found to have had its beginning in the creative act of an individual 
psyche. There is, of course, no other source for culture to come from, 

15 



16 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

for what culture is made of is but the raw stuff of experience, whether 
material or spiritual, transformed into culture by the creativeness 
of man. 1 An analysis of culture, if fully carried out, leads back to 
the individual psyche. 

The content of any particular psyche, on the other hand, comes 
from culture. No individual or group of individuals can ever origi- 
nate their culture it comes to them from without, in the process of 
education. 

In its constituent elements culture is psychological and, in the 
last analysis, comes from the individual. But as an integral entity, 
culture is cumulative, historical, extra-individual. It comes to the 
individual as part of his objective experience, just as do his experi- 
ences with nature and, like these, it is absorbed by him, thus becom- 
ing part of his psychic content. 

This double relationship between culture and psychology has led 
to two opposite tendencies in the interpretation of cultural phenom- 
ena. 

If culture is psychological in essence and individual in origin, it 
was argued, it should be possible to furnish an individual psycho- 
logical interpretation of culture, at any given time and place, with- 
out leaving any residue. The "great men" theorists of history often 
fell into this error, as did almost all systems of psychology, from the 
subjective analytical attempts of the classical associationists to the 
semi-experimental folk-psychologists of the Wundtian variety, and 
to the modern psychoanalysts. The evolutionary anthropolgists and 
sociologists were similarly at fault when they represented the culture 
of this or that tribe as determined by its psychological make-up. 

To the same category belongs a recent work on social psychology, 
in which the claim is made that all "explanations" (as distinguished 
from descriptions) in culture must rest in the individual, that the 
"causes" for cultural change can only be found in individuals. 

All of these ideologies fail to do justice to the cumulative aspect 
of culture; they ignore the fact that the individual or individuals of 
a group are ever at the mercy of the precedents, patterns, styles, 
fashions of culture. Nor do they pay due heed to the fact that in the 
life of every people historical factors come into play which, from the 

1 A branch is a bit of the physical environment. A branch used as a club is culture. 



PSYCHOLOGY AND CULTURE 17 

standpoint of that particular culture, must be classed as accidental. 
The classical examples of this are, of course, borrowed cultural fea- 
tures which come from other tribes and cultures. These features are 
patently independent, not alone of the psychology of the receipient 
group, but also of its culture. 

In sharp contrast to the preceding stand all those theorists who 
stress the historical and superindividual aspect of culture at the 
expense of individual and psychological factors. They assume that 
culture is wholly an objective, extra-individual entity, specific unto 
itself in nature and behavior, which imposes itself upon individuals 
who, at best, can only be said to reflect it. 

Among the evolutionists, the economic interpreters of history, 
comprising Marx and his followers, represent this point of view. To 
them culture is objective, social, historical, cumulative, dynamic, 
deterministic. To culture the chariot of history is harnessed. The 
individual is fleeting, passive, epi-phenomenal, causally irrelevant. 
He is sitting in the chariot spinning highfaluting theories about its 
progress while culture drives on. Here also belong the modern in- 
stitutional anthropologists and sociologists, although their fervor is 
less irrational and unreal. 

Those who think in this fashion do scant justice to the active 
participation of the individual in cultural growth. For while it is 
certainly true that the cultural content comes to the individual in a 
way that is external and objective, the individual does, after all, 
recreate what he receives. He does so unconsciously by dint of the 
very variability of his native endowment, as well as consciously in 
the overt acts of psychic originality. 

Even apart from this, however, the individual cannot be ex- 
plained away by culture, for his psychic content is the result of what 
might be called biographical selection. The specific cultural content 
which comes to the psyche is not the only thing that counts. There 
is also the chronological order of the coming, as well as the emotional 
setting in which each element of experience is received. 1 It is this 
difference in the order and the apperceptive conditions which con- 
stitutes a large part of what we call individual differences. If this 

1 By way of illustration, compare two individuals familiar with strikes and economic 
theory, but one lived through a strike first then studied economics, the other did the 
reverse. 



i8 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

were not so, individuals of approximately equal native endowment 
and similar cultural setting would also be similar when clothed in 
their cultural garb, whereas as a matter of fact, the opposite is 
obviously true. 

And finally, however objective and extraneous to the individual 
culture may be, it must after all be remembered that it ceases to 
count, unless it be potentially, as soon as the psychic channels 
through which it can be communicated are removed. Culture, 
whether spiritual or material, counts only to the extent to which it 
is operative through psychic channels. Cultural features that are 
neither intellectually nor practically used nor aesthetically appreci- 
ated, nor are even known to exist, do actually not exist, as culture, 
except potentially. 1 They thus become equivalent to the passive 
elements of the physical environment, such as metallic ores in the 
Stone Age, which, although objectively present, are yet culturally 
impotent because they are not reacted to or otherwise made use of. 

II. THE MOST GENERAL RELATION OF THE INDIVIDUAL 
TO SOCIETY 

Man, being part of culture, is also part of society, the carrier of 
culture. This circumstance has also led to an apparently paradoxical 
situation which gave rise to three different ideological solutions. 
Some held that man being the locus of sensory life was therefore the 
measure of all things, and that society was nothing but a grouping of 
individuals. All interpretations of social phenomena must therefore 
be sought in the processes of the individual psyche. 

Others insisted that man came into the world a social animal; 
that all there was to him was social; that man the individual was an 
abstraction; and that all social as well as individual phenomena had 
therefore to be interpreted in social terms. Still others pointed out 
that the whole question was a fictitious one, was what the Germans 
call a Scheinproblem, a "pseudo-problem," that the individual was 
society and society the individual, and that the very attempt to 
juxtapose the two was a bit of abstractionist futility. 

All three approaches can be shown to be erroneous. It is true 

1 Such may be the fate of the Bible in some modern communities or, in others, of a 
Wagnerian opera. 



PSYCHOLOGY AND CULTURE ig 

enough that man is the locus of psychic life and therefore represents 
what may be called the sensory level of social phenomena. At the 
same time, social determinants implied, for example, in motor habits 
or language, creep into the individual psyche surreptitiously and 
unconsciously. To disentangle the "pure" individual psyche from 
their meshes seems well-nigh impossible. Also, the effectiveness of 
some individuals as social factors of necessity molds and determines 
other individuals, so that the very significance and conspicuousness 
of some persons brings with it the passivity and relative nonimport- 
ance of others. 

Again, the extreme social determinists err in so far as they unduly 
simplify the realm of social forces faced by the individual. As a 
matter of fact this realm is highly complex, consisting of many in 
part contradictory social influences, some of which reinforce each 
other while others cancel out, allowing for the occasional emergence 
of the relatively detached or desocialized individual. 1 

Those, finally, who deny the very existence of the problem of the 
relation of the individual to society are once more guilty of an ab- 
stractionist simplification. This relation is not a problem only in 
the sense that it is many problems, for the relation of the individual 
to the social varies both with the social situation and with the char- 
acter and state of the individual. 

This reflection brings us to our next topic. 

III. LEVELS IN THE RELATIONS OF THE INDIVIDUAL TO SOCIETY 

Among the bonds that tie the individual to the group, some are 
complex, others simple, some categoric, others loose. This can be 
demonstrated by analyzing one of the simplest forms of social situ- 
ation, namely, a crowd consisting of relatively like-minded individu- 
als in physical or immediate contact and subjected to a common 
stimulus. In this case the psychic content and the resulting reac- 

1 Perhaps the most significant contrast between modern and primitive communities 
lies in this domain. Relatively speaking, the social forces or pressures in primitive 
society work uniformly, so as to affect all, and along parallel lines, as it were; while in 
modern society, owing to the multiplication of social subgroupings and the specialization 
of functions, these social forces or pressures constitute a complicated and intertwining 
network. As a result the individual is at times bewildered, but he may also slip through 
the meshes unscathed and relatively desocialized. 



20 TEE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

tions of the individuals constituting the crowd are so uniform that 
the crowd does indeed appear but as a magnified individual and the 
individual but as a reflection of the crowd. For this reason also, 
while the crowd does not possess an existential soul of its own, it 
acts as if it had one, and if, indeed, it were assumed that it did, our 
interpretation of the crowd would not be modified thereby. On the 
other hand, an inspection of the individual psyche as revealed in 
crowd phenomena makes possible a fine insight into the cruder as- 
pects of the original nature of man, for all that is truly individual and 
specific, such as the higher intellectual and spiritual functions, are 
for the time being in abeyance. 1 

At the other extreme is the lone thinker solving an intellectual 
problem in an abstract subject the theory of numbers, for example. 
To him the social world is but an academic assumption the presence 
or absence of which in no way affects the task in which he is con- 
cerned. 

Between these two extremes lies a vast series of levels of social- 
ization which allow of more careful conceptualization and more de- 
liberate study than have heretofore been accorded them. 2 

1 The reason for the oft-observed phenomenon that the individual in a crowd is, 
as it were, pulled down to a lower level of biological life is, of course, to be sought in the 
fact that the emotional and instinctive factors are much older and basic in our make-up 
and constitute for that reason the common denominator in any group of individuals. 
If we were to assume, for argument's sake, that man is basically sapiens, that the in- 
tellectual elements are genetically older, and that they were in later evolutionary stages 
relatively thinly overlaid with irrational and emotional elements, then a crowd made up 
of such individuals would indeed create an intellectual superman. It would then think 
with its head, not its "bowels," 

I may add, in passing, that the above remarks are not calculated to dispose of high 
minded crowds. This occurs when an idealistic cue is given to a crowd under favorable 
apperceptive conditions. Instance : an appeal for funds in war time. 

3 Samples of such levels may be discerned in the following situations: a crowd in the 
narrowest sense, comprising like-minded persons in intimate contact and subjected to a 
common emotional stimulus; a crowd of less homogeneous consistency, or one subjected 
to a less powerful stimulus; then a crowd in a wider sense, such as the readers of the 
New York Times or the Dearborn Independent, when subjected to a "typical" editorial; 
then a group of individuals in the seclusion of their respective universities engaged in 
the study of the social sciences, who, while relatively detached, are yet subject to a 
variety of irrational determinants; and finally, students of mathematics or celestial 
mechanics, each isolated not only physically but conceptually, alone, unemotional, de- 
tached, desocialized, homo sapiens par excellence. 



PSYCHOLOGY AND CULTURE 21 

IV. PSYCHOLOGY AND CULTURE, FROM THE METHODOLOGICAL 

STANDPOINT 

The psychic unity of mankind lies, first of all, in the realm of the 
original nature of man. In the present state of psychological (or 
shall we say biological?) knowledge, we may be able to discern but 
dimly the outlines of this original nature. It is certain, nevertheless, 
that the major range of psychic unity springs from this source. The 
sensory qualities of man, his amenability to the gastric, sexual, and 
propagatory urges, his gregariousness and sociability, his pugnacity 
and playfulness, his inventiveness and inertia, his limited but withal 
marvelous capacity to " think straight, " his love of work and enjoy- 
ment of virtuosity, his vanity and love of power, and any number of 
other traits, are certainly lodged in the original nature of man, either 
actually or potentially (whatever this may be). For this reason man 
everywhere, at all times and in all cultural stages, is so much the 
same. This is psychic unity. This also is the domain of psychology, 
Bastian may have been exasperatingly vague in his treatment of the 
Elementargedanken, but at bottom he was right, for his "elemental 
ideas" of mankind is but another name for psychic unity. 

Psychic unity expresses itself in cultural features of universal 
distribution. Universal features can be interpreted psychologically. 
Take the belief in spirits and magic. Psychologists of different 
schools have tackled it the associationists, the folk- psychologists, 
the psychoanalysts and each group has contributed valuable bits 
of interpretation. 

Features that are general but not universal constitute a more 
difficult problem. Take, for instance, the clan, or the mother-in-law 
taboo, or secret societies. All are common, none universal. Does 
this mean that psychology must be discarded and history applied if 
an explanation is to be sought? Not necessarily. There may be, in 
fact, there are, perfectly good psychological or socio-psychological 
reasons for all three, but such reasons are not always categorical; 
they may constitute a trend, a tendency, without being an impera- 
tive. Whenever such situations occur where psychological and histor- 
ical factors are blended, the methodological difficulties are, of course, 
tremendous, and if psychological interpretations are at all applied, 
they are usually doomed to remain hypothetical. 



22 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

Thus we finally reach cases where the features to be explained 
are in a true sense peculiarities which occur only in a particular tribe, 
district, nation. Cultural traits of this sort cannot be explained psy- 
chologically here history claims its own. All we can say is that 
under no cultural conditions can features appear which would con- 
tradict psychology, but the specific features that do appear can only 
by accounted for historically. 

V. HISTORICAL EXPLANATIONS AND PSYCHOLOGY 

In view of the above considerations it is often assumed that 
historical explanations preclude psychology. This is erroneous. To 
illustrate: Modern culture differs from primitive culture, one primi- 
tive culture differs from another, a modern national culture, that of 
France, differs from another, that of Germany. The explanation for 
all this is historical. So much being granted, there is still ample room 
for psychological illumination. The differentia of modern and primi- 
tive culture are determined by history, this gives the background 
for what may be called the modern mind and the primitive mind; 
these in turn may be used as a basis for psychological interpretations. 
In other words, there is such a thing as the "primitive mind/ 7 just as 
there is a French mind and a German mind, even though the ex- 
planation for such "minds" must be sought in history, not in biology. 1 

VI. INDIVIDUAL-PSYCHOLOGICAL AND CULTURAL ANALYSIS 
AS HEURISTIC TOOLS 

In conclusion, a word about individual analysis and cultural 
study as tools in the ever fascinating quest of penetrating more 

1 This may be illustrated by the application of two concepts recently introduced 
by psychoanalysts: introversion and extra version. Introverts are those who solve their 
problems and adjustments to the world in subjective terms, extra verts, those whose 
terms are objective or conventional. Some psychoanalysts extend the use of these con- 
cepts from individuals to societies or nations; they speak of an introverted Germany, 
an extraverted England or America. Now this cannot mean that there are more 
extraverts born in England or America, more introverts in Germany. Nevertheless, 
the terms introverted or extraverted nation may not be absurd. In an introverted 
nation, introversion is at a premium, therefore born extraverts will be driven into 
introvert compensation; in an extravert nation the reverse will be true. An introvert 
or extravert nation may be the product of a purely historic process, but, as introvert 
or extravert, it has a typical psychic orientation. The rest follows. (The possiblity 
that the concepts "introvert" and "extravert" may be invalid or imperfectly circum- 
scribed need not concern us here.) 



PSYCHOLOGY AND CULTURE 23 

deeply into the nature of the individual psyche and into the essence 
of culture. Here, the present cultural situation is our laboratory. 

We are told that the burden of culture is becoming too much for 
the individual, that the ever increasing complexity of culture com- 
bined with the modern urge for self-expression and personal freedom 
create a psychological quagmire, impassable for the average human. 
Hence, an increase in criminality, suicides, neuroses. At this point 
psychoanalysis steps in and, out of its intensive explorations of the 
struggle of original nature against the impositions of culture, there 
emerges fresh illumination of the very kernel of original nature. 
Whatever may be said in criticism of Freudian and other such 
mechanisms, the fact remains that we know more today about the 
urge of sex, about repressions, conflicts, compensations, and sublima- 
tions than we did yesterday, and we hope for even a richer harvest 
tomorrow. Original nature on the warpath proves a fertile field for 
psychological exploration. 

And there is a parallel situation in culture. While the individual 
remained subdued by institutional norms, the double task of culture 
of preserving the old and ushering in the new seemed to take care of 
itself smoothly enough. Now comes the call for a revaluation of 
values. Less of the old more of the new; less inertia more prog- 
ress; less institutionalism more creative individualism; less cul- 
tural norm more personal self-expression. Under the stress and 
strain of these new demands culture itself is developing a complex. 
Should it give way, who will be the healer? To the thinker, mean- 
while, new vistas are opened toward a deeper understanding of the 
nature of culture. How much mobility will it stand? What methods 
could be employed to increase its plasticity? Will the new receipe 
for culture be an improvement upon the old? Thus cultural rebirth 
may furnish clinical data to the social pathologist. 



CULTURE AND CULTURAL TRENDS 



ROBERT E. PARK 
University of Chicago 



ABSTRACT 

Culture and Cultural Trends. Cultural traits fall into (a) form, those which are 
external and visible as arts, ceremonial, and tools; and (Z>) content, those which are deep 
and inaccessible as memories, tradition, and unorganized social experience. Similar to 
the distinction between form and content is the contrast between technique and mores. 
Folkways, which with preliteratc people are practically identical with mores, under re- 
flection and experimentation lose their institutional character and become technique. 
Just as culture emphasizes the mores, civilization tends to be applied to what is general 
and rational in culture, that is, to technique. The harmony between the ideal and the 
material aspects of culture is far more characteristic of a society like that of the Eskimo 
than of modern metropolitan life, because changes in our material culture are moving 
at a tempo with which our ideals and our institutions are not able to keep pace. Cul- 
tural trends are those changes taking place in the mores, in law, in public opinion, and 
in philosophy in the struggle to bring these into harmonious relations with social con- 
ditions. 

I. CULTURE AS FORM AND AS CONTENT 

In a paper published in 1911 in Nature, W. H. Rivers sought to 
distinguish between those elements of culture which are embodied 
in external, visible, and tangible objects, and those other less obvious, 
less formal aspects of culture which he called "social structure." 

I shall not attempt to define social structure as Rivers uses the 
term. The conception is, at best, a dubious one. Interesting and 
important, however, is the distinction he has tried to make between 
elements or aspects of a culture which may be transmitted, as he 
says, by mere contact, and those other cultural complexes which re- 
main in the background of the folk mind, invisible but active, color- 
ing and modifying the overt behavior of the people of whom they 
are, perhaps, a forgotten heritage. 

We find in Oceania .... people wearing European clothes and European 
ornaments, using European utensils, and even European weapons when they 
fight; we find them holding the beliefs and practicing the ritual of a European 
religion; we find them speaking a European language, often even among them- 
selves, and yet investigation shows that much of their social structure remains 
thoroughly native and uninfluenced, not only in its general form, but often even 

24 



CULTURE AND CULTURAL TRENDS 25 

in its minute details. The external influence has swept away the whole material 
culture, so that objects of native origin are manufactured only to sell to tourists; 
it has substituted a wholly new religion and destroyed every material, if not 
every moral, vestige of the old ; it has caused great modification and degeneration 
of the old language; and yet it may have left the social structure in the main un- 
touched. And the reasons for this are clear. Most of the essential social struc- 
ture of a people lies so below the surface it is so literally the foundation of the 
whole life of the people, that it is not seen; it is not obvious, but can only be 
reached by patient and laborious exploration. 1 

It is always easier to transmit our language than our ideas. It 
is less difficult to inculcate the forms of a religious cult than it is to 
translate the religious experiences in which these religious practices 
originated. It is easier to sell a sewing machine in China than to 
establish a parliamentary form of government. It is easier, in short, 
to introduce among any people tools or technical devices the use- 
fulness of which is obvious than to transplant an alien institution 
which has had a history and embodies in its motives and its structure 
the accumulated experience of successive generations. 

The most striking illustration of the sudden and successful adop- 
tion of an alien culture is Japan. And yet those observers who have 
had an opportunity to know Japan intimately assure us that as yet 
European culture has merely changed the exterior of Japanese life. 
Japan has taken over the science, the technique, and many of the 
external forms of European culture, but is, perhaps, less disposed 
today than it was sixty years ago to adopt the mores of the occi- 
dental world. 

The adoption of Western civilization was not nearly such an easy matter as 
unthinking persons imagined. And it is quite evident that the mental readjust- 
ments, effected at a cost which remains to be told, have given good results only 
along directions in which the race has always shown capacities of special kinds. 
Thus, the appliances of Western industrial invention have worked admirably 
in Japanese hands, have produced excellent results in those crafts at which the 
nation had been skilful, in other and quainter ways, for ages. There has been 
no transformation nothing more than the turning of old abilities into new 
and larger channels. 2 

In one limited sense, Western art has influenced Japanese literature and 
drama; but the character of the influence proves the racial differences to which I 

1 W. H. R. Rivers, "The Ethnological Analysis of Culture," in Nature, LXXXVII 
(1911), 358-60. 

2 Lafcadio Hearn, Kokoro (Boston: Houghton Mifflin Co., 1896), pp. 9-10. 



26 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

refer. European plays have been reshaped for the Japanese stage, and European 
novels rewritten for Japanese readers. But a literal version is rarely attempted, 
for the original incidents, thoughts, and emotions would be unintelligible to 
the average reader or playgoer. Plots are adopted, sentiments and incidents are 
totally transformed. "The New Magdalen" becomes a Japanese girl who mar* 
ried an Eta. Victor Hugo's Les Mistrables becomes a tale of the Japanese civil 
war, and Enjolras a Japanese student. There have been a few rare exceptions, 
including the marked success of a literal translation of the Sorrows of Werther* 

This does not mean that changes in Japanese life are not pro- 
found. It does mean that the Japanese ethos, rooted in temperament 
and reinforced by tradition, will, like any other living organism, per- 
sist, using the changed conditions to further and fortify its continued 
existence in a changed environment. The external adventitious ele- 
ments of its culture will change first and change more rapidly. What 
Rivers calls the structure of Japanese national life will change also, 
but more slowly. 

The contrast which Rivers draws between the changing, super- 
ficial aspect of culture and its relatively unchanging core, seems to 
reduce itself finally to the contrast between form and content. Those 
traits which are external and visible are form; those which are deep 
and inaccessible are content. We know what the external aspects of 
a culture are: they are its arts, its ceremonial, and its tools. But 
what of the content; in what, precisely, does this thing we call con- 
tent consist? It consists, apparently, in memories, that is to say, 
in tradition; in a relatively unorganized social experience; something 
which is, at any rate, less structure than will. 2 

Rivers' discussion of blended cultures is concerned with the proc- 
esses by which cultural traits are transmitted horizontally from one 
cultural group to another. But cultures are transmitted vertically 
from one generation to another in the same cultural group. The 
process is probably much the same, but the results are different. 
Children seem to take over intuitively and without resistance just 
those elements of a foreign culture which an adult alien finds most 

1 Lafcadio Hearn, Kokoro (Boston: Houghton Mifflin Co., 1896), pp. 10-11. 

2 This will is probably related to the libido of the psychoanalyst and to the elan 
mtale of Bergson. It is anything you please, so long as the term is used to describe a fact, 
and is more than the name of a mystical and metaphysical entity. 



CULTURE AND CULTURAL TRENDS 27 

difficult to understand and assimilate. Children do not inherit the 
cultural complexes of their parents, and when children of immigrants 
grow up in the country of their adoption they inevitably take over 
all the accents, the inflections, the local cultural idioms of the native 
population. This is true of the Chinese in America, even though they 
are reared as most of them are in a ghetto. Most of the native 
sons among the Chinese in California are outrageously American in 
their manners and in their sentiments. It is only in later life, if at 
all, that they revert to the ancestral tradition and acquire a second- 
ary racial loyalty. 

If it turns out as it frequently does that the culture of the 
second generation seems a little thin and superficial as compared 
with that of the first, it is due, no doubt, to the fact that this second 
generation, having lost or abandoned the older cultural heritages, is 
not quite in possession of the new. In the long run this undoubtedly 
affects American life as a whole. It manifests itself, I suspect, in the 
proverbial restlessness of the Amercan; in the extraordinary extent 
of family disorganization, i.e., divorce, desertion, as well as in the 
amount of juvenile delinquency and crime. 

For this and other reasons, the distinction between the form and 
the content of cultures, which Rivers found useful in his studies of 
his primitive folk, is even more important in the study of conflicts 
and fusions of racial and national cultures in our cosmopolitan and 
contemporary life. 

The significant thing about the whole matter is that, with the 
fusion and blending of culture, form and content tend to fall apart, 
nd gain each a more or less independent existence. The signi- 
ficance of this will appear later, when we come to define the changes 
we have called trends. 

II. TECHNIQUE AND THE MORES 

The distinction between form and content, as applied to the 
analysis of cultures, has a parallel and an analogy in the contrast 
between technique and the mores. 

Among primitive peoples, what Sumner calls "folkways" seem 
to have been identical, or nearly so, with what he later describes as 



28 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

mores. Folkways are not merely ways of doing all the common 
things of life as they have become defined in custom and trans- 
mitted through tradition, but they are the "right" ways. 

There is a right way to catch game, to win a wife, to make one's self appear, 
to cure disease, to honor ghosts, to treat comrades or strangers, to behave when 
a child is born, on the warpath, in council, and so on in all cases which can arise. 
The ways are defined on the negative side, that is, by taboos. The "right" way 
is the way which the ancestors used, and which has been handed down. The 
tradition is its own warrant. It is not held subject to verification by experience. 
The notion of right is in the folkways. It is not outside of them, of independent 
origin, and brought to them to test them. In the folkways, whatever is, is right. 1 

Folkways, however, become in the course of time the object of 
reflection. Crude and vague notions of societal welfare grow up in 
regard to them and serve to explain and justify them. Then, accord- 
ing to Sumner, folkways assume the character of mores. The mores 
are not themselves a philosophy of life, but they hold a philosophy 
of life in solution. But folkways are not merely customary ways of 
doing things. They are, at the same time, devices and contrivances 
for getting things done and for doing them effectively. The folk- 
ways are practices which embody the elements of technique. Prac- 
tice leads inevitably to experiment and then to reflection on these 
experiments. Out of these reflections we get eventually science, i.e., 
natural science. When science has explained the arts, ceremonies, 
and practical devices by which we do things and get them done, 
these arts, ceremonies, and other devices assume the character of 
technique. 

The effect of this experimentation upon the folkways is to secu- 
larize them; to take them out from under the mores that is to say', 
out from under the control of custom and the group and put them 
at the service of the individual man. In this way, they cease to be a 
form of social ritual and become a kind of tool, a means to an end, 
rather than an end in themselves. 

This process of secularization begins with simple and elementary 
practices like sowing and reaping, the making of gins and traps, the 
manufacture of tools. It extends itself eventually to all the activities 
of life. 

1 W. G. Sumner, Folkways (Ginn and Company, 1906), 28. 



CULTURE AND CULTURAL TRENDS 29 

Furthermore, when customary practices are made intelligible in 
the manner described, they are more easily transmitted from one 
cultural group to another. They can be bartered and sold in the 
marketplace, they lose their institutional character, and become part 
of that general culture of mankind which we ordinarily call civiliza- 
tion. 

The history of modern civilization, viewed from one point of 
view, is the history of the continued secularization and rationaliza- 
tion of human activities, in the course of which ritual has become 
technique. 

The natural sciences, at least in the occidental world, have long 
since entirely lost their local and national character. Philosophy and 
art, on the other hand, still reflect temperament and the historical 
experience of nations and of peoples of whose cultures they are a 
peculiarly intimate expression. 

The effect of these international labors has been to destroy the clearly 
marked differences of national thought. At least in the domain of science, the 
peculiarities of the French, the German, and the English schools are rapidly 
disappearing. The characteristics of national thought still exist; but in order to 
find them in the present age we should have to study the deeper philosophical 
reasonings, the general literature, and the artistic efforts of the three nations. 
.... The establishment of an observatory or a laboratory in our age lays under 
contribution almost every civilized country in the world, and the most inter- 
national of sciences that of electricity fixes its units by the names of dis- 
coverers of many countries. 1 

Those cultural traits which have so completely lost their local, 
tribal, and national character that they have ceased to be expressive, 
^ave, at the same time, lost their character as culture. They may be 
regarded as a part of nature. Civilization, on the other hand, is a 
more abstract conception than culture. Civilization includes both 
technique and the mores. 

Civilization, as we ordinarily use the term, is not a local phe- 
nomenon in the same sense in which that is true of culture. It is not 
a term which describes what is individual and unique in the life of 
races or peoples. Civilization is the term we apply to those aspects 
of culture which have been generalized, rationalized, and are gen- 

1 John Theodore Merz, European Thought in the Nineteenth Century (London: 
William Black wood and Sons, 1907), I, 305. 



30 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

erally intelligible. So far as there is any distinction in the use of the 
terms, civilization emphasizes technique as culture emphasizes 
mores. 

It has been a frequent observation of students of society that 
social groups, although consisting of individuals each with his own 
private opinions and purposes, frequently acts as if each group had a 
mind of its own, a group mind. We may, as James Harvey Robinson 
says we must, have to "reconcile ourselves to novel and revolution- 
ary conceptions of the mind, for it is clear that the older philoso- 
phers, whose works still determine our current views, had a very 
superficial notion of the subject with which they dealt." 1 

Without venturing to discuss the question as to whether the 
group mind is "real" entity in any such sense as the mind of the 
individual is supposed to be, we may at least use the analogy to de- 
scribe the way in which social groups do actually behave. 

We are already familiar with such expressions as the "rural 
mind," "the urban mind," "the medieval mind," "mind in the mak- 
ing," etc. What we look for in the materials which the study of 
cultures offers us is something which enables us not merely to esti- 
mate their mentality, but to know their minds, using the term 
"mind" as Henry Osborne Taylor does when he speaks of the 
"medieval mind," or as Bliss Perry does when he speaks of the 
American mind. 

That is what is meant by saying that cultural materials must be 
expressive. We can only know the minds of peoples, as we know a 
work of art, in so far as we are able to re-create, in our own minds, 
the experiences which have made them what they are. * 

Lafcadio Hearn, who was an adept in the art of "feeling him- 
self," as the Germans say, into the lives of strange people, speaks 
with authority on this topic: 

Sympathy is limited by comprehension. We may sympathize to the same 
degree that we understand. One may imagine that he sympathizes with a Jap- 
anese or a Chinese, but the sympathy can never be real to more than a small 
extent outside of the simplest phases of common emotional life, those phases in 
which child and man are at one. The more complex feelings of the Oriental have 
been composed by the combinations of experiences, ancestral and individual, 

1 James Harvey Robinson, The Mind in the Making (New York: Harper & Bros., 
, P- 36- 



CULTURE AND CULTURAL TRENDS 31 

which have had no really precise correspondence in Western Life, and which we 
can therefore not fully know. For converse reasons, the Japanese cannot, even 
though they would, give Europeans their best sympathy. 1 

Whether the distinction between technique and the mores, with 
which I started out, or the distinction between culture and civiliza- 
tion, with which I ended, are wholly tenable or not does not greatly 
matter for the purposes of this paper. It is at least true that civiliza- 
tion is the broader term, and that within the wide embrace of a 
civilization, local, tribal, and even national, cultures may be found 
to exist. It is perhaps only when we consider civilization and culture 
in this relation to each other that the significance of the distinction 
between them becomes impressive. Using the terms with this defini- 
tion, we might say of America today that it has, to be sure, a civiliza- 
tion, but not a culture. This is, in fact, pretty nearly what Horace 
Kallen does say in defense of his program of cultural pluralism. 

Decidedly, the older America, whose voice and whose spirit were New Eng- 
land, has, by virtue of business, of communications, of the immigrant, gone 
beyond recall. Americans of British stock still are prevailingly the artists and 
thinkers of the land, but they work each for himself, without common vision 
or ideals. They have no ethos any more. The older tradition has passed from a 
life into a memory, and the newer one, so far as it had an Anglo-Saxon base, is 
holding its own beside more and more formidable competitors, the expression 
in appropriate form of the national inheritances of the various populations con- 
centrated in various states of the Union, populations of whom their national 
self-consciousness is perhaps the chief spiritual asset, as their labor-power is 
their chief economic asset. 2 

I am not defending the view here expressed. I do not accept it; 
\ merely quote it. But the fact that this view has been uttered and 
defended by a mind of unusual perspicuity and intelligence is itself 
significant. 

in. CULTURAL TRENDS 

The unifying element in every cultural complex is, in the lan- 
guage of Clark Wissler, "a core of ideas and beliefs, actuating a peo- 
ple and in a large measure controlling their career." The core of 
ideas and beliefs and the material objects in which these ideas and 

x Lafcadio Hearn, Kokoro (Boston: Houghton Mifflin, 1896), pp. 11-12. 

3 Horace Kallen, Culture and Democracy in the United States (Boni and Liveright), 
p. 105. 



32 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

beliefs are embodied mutually interact upon one another to pro- 
duce a harmonious whole. We are constantly remaking the world in 
accordance with our desires, but that world, once created, inevitably 
reacts back upon our desires to reshape them and define them in 
conformity with itself. 

"Our experience with the world/' says Wissler, "indicates that 
whenever we find sharp contrasts in such homely affairs as housing 
and feeding, we are certain to find equal, if not even sharper, con- 
trasts in beliefs, social ways, ideals, and ethics, and in fact all mental 
attitudes toward things of whatever sort." 

This admirable harmony between the ideal and material, be- 
tween the subjective and objective aspects of culture, is probably a 
good deal more characteristic of the Eskimo, which is the illustration 
Wissler uses, than it is to take an extreme case of the hobo, or 
in fact of any other representative of our modern, cosmopolitan, 
urban life. Between the material objects of the hobo's cultural world 
and his beliefs and wishes the contrast is so great as to amount to 
conflict. The same is true, however, of almost every other class in 
modern society. 

It is not merely true that the hobo as an individual is not always 
able to make ends meet, but the hobo's ideals of life are out of har- 
mony with the general scheme of things. His life lacks form, and he 
is restless, migratory, and unsettled in his mind. 

But this is characteristic of every class in modern society. Even 
our most solid citizens have a sense of insecurity which probably 
rests finally upon a sense of incongruity between our material cul- 
ture and our aims and ideals of life. , 

The reason seems to be that the changes in our material culture 
are moving at a tempo with which our ideas and ideals have not 
been able to keep pace. This lack of congruity manifests itself not 
merely in our art and in our religion, but in our politics. Our institu- 
tions we hear it repeated on all sides are out of touch with life. 
Our art and architecture, like the hobo's career, is free, but lacks 
form. As Von Ogden Vogt has put it, our art is nondescript. 

The arts constitute the description of the world as an age or a people ap- 
prehends it. The spiritual life of a time is depicted with unescapable exactness 
in its artistry. A spiritual movement that does not find expression in the arts 



CULTURE AND CULTURAL TRENDS 33 

cannot attain self-consciousness or dominance or survival. An age or a people 
that does not reach any self-realization or any unity of thought or feeling that 
breaks forth into artistic expression is nondescript. 1 

And then he adds: 

Rebels, prophets, protestants, are in every time and place, but if they are 
in the majority, the community is nondescript, and the voices of the arts are 
mute, for they have no great thing to say * 

This is a nondescript age in which we live. The old isolations 
within which the older cultures grew up have broken down. "No 
race can again form so separate a culture and artistry as that of Siam 
or Japan. The world is one as never before. And it is nondescript 
as never before." 3 

On the other hand, religion, as it has found institutional expres- 
sion in the church, is likewise out of touch with modern life and 
thought. Science and practical life have moved on and left the church 
with its creeds behind. 

The crisis in the religious world has been brought about by the failure of 
existing religion to adapt itself to the two outstanding facts in our civilization 

science and democracy Of these two, science is the more outstanding 

and dominant. It is the foundation of our views of life and of the universe, as 
well as of our material progress, and so it has largely created the conditions 
which have favored the rise of modern democracy. Yet the maladjustment of 
religion with science remains pronounced. 4 

Roscoe Pound has made similar observations in regard to our 
political and judicial institutions: 

To understand the administration of criminal justice in American cities 
today we must first perceive the problems of administration of justice in a homo- 
geneous, pioneer, primarily agricultural community of the first half of the nine- 
teenth century, and the difficulties involved in meeting those problems with 
the legal institutions and legal doctrines inherited or received from seventeenth- 
century England. We must then perceive the problems of administration of 
justice in a modern, heterogeneous, urban, industrial community, and the diffi- 
culties involved in meeting those problems with the legal and judicial machinery 
inherited or received from England and adapted and given new and fixed shape 
for pioneer rural America. 5 

1 Von Ogden Vogt,Art and Religion (New Haven: Yale University Press, 1921), p. 9. 

3 Ibid., p. ii. 3 Ibid., p. 16. 

* Charles A. Ell wood, The Reconstruction of Religion (New York: The Macmillan 
Company, 1922), p. 2. 

s Roscoe Pound, Criminal Justice in Cleveland (Cleveland: The Cleveland Founda- 
tion, 1922), p. 590. 



34 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

Disease has been described as an evidence and an effect of a 
lack of adjustment between the organism and its environment. So- 
cial disorganization may, with probably equal justice to the facts, 
be described as an evidence of conflict or incongruity between the 
subjective and objective aspects of culture, between the group mind 
and the instrumentalities through which that mind acts. 

It is the thesis of this paper that what we ordinarily call cultural 
trends are changes that take place in the mores, in law, in public 
opinion, and in philosophy in the struggle to bring these into some 
sort of consistent and harmonious relations with social conditions 
and, as we so often say, actual life. 

There is a fashion in public opinion, in law, in the mores, and in 
culture generally, as there is in all things that express the life and 
the will of the social group. The general direction which fashion 
takes is what I should like to describe as a cultural trend. 

The classic illustration of such a trend is Dicey's account of the 
changes in public opinion in England in the period beginning with 
the end of the eighteenth century and ending with the twentieth. 1 
Dicey speaks of law and public opinion, but the public opinion that 
he describes includes what Sumner calls the mores. It is the change 
in the mores, as they were reflected in law, with which he is mainly 
concerned. 

What he says, in effect, is that from the end of the eighteenth 
century to the end of the nineteenth, changes in English law, with, 
to be sure, many divagations and shif tings, took one general direc- 
tion. On the whole, these changes represented a shift from a more 
individualistic to a more communistic conception of government 
and human relations. The direction which this change took was a 
trend. 

Similar changes have been taking place in our own society. 
Without intending to do so, without clearly realizing that they are 
doing so, our legislatures, in seeking to regulate the railways and 
other public or quasi-public corporations, have been steadily under- 
mining the conception of private property inherited from England. 
The labor organizations, in their struggles with the employers of 
labor, have worked, consciously and unconsciously, in the same 

1 A. V. Dicey, Law and Public Opinion in England. 



CULTURE AND CULTURAL TRENDS 35 

direction. The latest and most radical expression of the labor move- 
ment is embodied in the phrase "citizenship in industry." Citizen- 
ship in industry is an assertion of labor's right, in conjunction with 
its employers, to fix wages and define the conditions under which 
industry should be carried on. 

The expression, "boring from within/' is simply a description and 
a rationalization of the process by which labor has actually under- 
mined the institution of property upon which the employer's right 
to control the industry rests. 

If now we consider causes, the systematic character of these 
changes and the apparent failure of both parties to understand their 
fundamentally subversive character seem to preclude the notion that 
they were the result of design, or consciously intended by the persons 
who brought them about. The causes were deeper and more imper- 
sonal. 

When we review what actually has taken place in the nineteenth 
century, we observe that with the growth of cities the multiplica- 
tion of the means of transportation and communication, and the 
consequent division of labor, the interdependence of individuals and 
of peoples has been vastly extended. And this extension of the divi- 
sion of labor, with the consequent economic interdependence which 
it involves, has undermined not only the independence of the work- 
ingman, but of every one of us. Under conditions of modern life 
the workingman bears, or did at one time, most of the economic 
costs of the fluctuations of industrial production. Every new indus- 
trial machine puts the hard-won skill of a group of workers on the 
scrap-heap. Every fluctuation in trade threw large numbers of men 
out of work. With this increasing extension of economic interde- 
pendence of all individuals and classes, it was inevitable that eventu- 
ally laws should be passed that recognized this interdependence and 
sought to control it. 

In general, one may say that changes in what Rivers calls the 
less material culture always and inevitably reflect changes in the 
more material culture. In other words, technique and the mores are 
so related that any change in the former inevitably brings corre- 
sponding changes in the latter. The relation is, however, probably 
not reversible. This is, perhaps, one explanation of the fact as well 



36 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

as the illusion of progress, progress being a cultural trend in which we 
see social conditions constantly in process of amelioration in one 
direction while they are just as steadily deteriorating in others. 

Nothing inspiring or uplifting seems to follow from the cleavage 
I have attempted to point out between the core of the cultural com- 
plex and its expression in physical objects; there is no immediate 
practical significance, either, to the fact that what we call cultural 
trends seem to arise out of the struggle for coherence among ele- 
ments, subjective and objective, within the cultural complex. 

It is significant, however, that these distinctions have in every 
case arisen, not merely in an attempt to analyze primitive cultures, 
but in the effort to deal practically with the problems of racial and 
cultural conflicts as they manifest themselves, among people with 
whom we are brought in everyday contact, within the limits of what 
Henry James calles the "American scene." 

What does strike one as hopeful, is that further investigation on 
the lines here indicated, will throw a new and a more searching light 
upon the whole cultural process, so that in the future we may hope 
to study it empirically, rather than discuss it philosophically, as this 
paper has been compelled to do. 



THE SUBJECTIVE ASPECT OF CULTURE 



ELLSWORTH PARIS 
University of Chicago 



ABSTRACT 

The present trend of opposition to instinctivism implies a rejection of evolutionism 
as a tool of analysis for social psychologists. Behaviorism and all similar efforts to 
interpret personality in terms of reflexes involves the unjustifiable assumption that 
culture is added to the human animal and that the two can separately be investigated. 
Heredity and cultural environment are not separate forces, they are merely tools of 
analysis. They are justifiable abstractions but are too often conceived as independent 
forces. Social psychology, defined as the study of the subjunctive aspect of culture, 
includes the study of groups and the study of individuals. The most important fields 
in which data can, at present, be found seem to lie in the realms of ethnology, history, 
and biography. 

The efforts of astronomers to signal to the planet Mars on the 
occasion of the last opposition, were, unfortunately, not rewarded 
with success, but the Martians, who are more advanced than we, 
did succeed in landing an expedition of scholars on the North 
American continent. In a recent conversation with one of the mem- 
bers of the expedition, I found it very difficult to convince him that 
we still have a large group of students of psychology who seek to 
explain and understand human nature by collecting anecdotes about 
the ants, bees, and the wild oxen of Patagonia. The Martian was at 
first incredulous. He, too, had been interested in zoology and had 
studied the fishes, insects, birds, and quadrupeds of the earth, but 
he had thought it necessary, before venturing any conclusions about 
the terrestrial featherless bipeds who have built cities, erected mon- 
asteries, built palaces and temples, and practiced institutionalized 
torture, to draw the facts from the behavior of the human animals 
themselves. The Martian was very insistent. No aspect of life, said 
he, was so interesting or so various in its manifestations as the 
human animals, and none interested him more. But the behavior of 
the different varieties of the species were so different that he felt he 
was under the necessity of spending some months to get at the facts 
of human life in their variety before he could venture any conclu- 

37 



38 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

sions. I tried to explain to him that we had been at it more or less 
continuously for sixty-six generations. He was amazed. "Is it possi- 
ble?" was all he could say. 

I tried to defend our clan. "You see, we have evolution, we have 
instincts, we have neurones, synapses, and reflexes." Evolution he 
had heard that word, but he had been under the impression that it 
referred to anatomical structures and their relations, which I had 
to admit. "Then how can you apply it to societal sequences?" Well, I 
squirmed, it was a beautiful idea, and we thought we should like to 
apply it to human society. "But that does not appear defensible," he 
said. "It seems, pardon me, almost indolent to take over the gen- 
eralizations of one field and apply them in advance to another." I 
quickly changed the subject. 

"But we have human instincts?" The celestial visitor smiled. 
"What are you trying to put over one me, anyhow? I have been in 
the libraries long enough to know that the instincts of the human 
being are listed in the most chaotic fashion. Three eminent writers 
agree in listing just one human instinct, one gives just two, several 
give four, others ten, some twelve, sixteen, twenty-three, and on to 
a hundred or more." 

"Well," I conceded, "there has been a recent revolt from the 
instinct doctrine, but at least we have behaviorism." 

"It sounds bad to call a scientific doctrine an ism, but go ahead." 

Behaviorism, I tried to explain, is a method of understanding 
and investigating human nature by external observation. 

"But what about the imagination can they observe that?" 

"Hush!" I said, "you must not mention imagination, some be- 
haviorist might hear you it is taboo." 

"You mean that they do not try to consider the internal aspect 
of men?" 

"0, yes, the neuroses and synapses." 

"Ah! that sounds interesting," he said. "I have often wished 
there were some technique for observing the working of the nervous 
system, for obviously it is of the utmost importance in behavior. 
And what technique have the behaviorists developed for observing 
the working of the nerve currents?" 

"Well, we have books and pictures about them." 



THE SUBJECTIVE ASPECT OF CULTURE 39 

"But I mean the working of the synapses!" 

"Well, we dissect the brain." 

"0, I see, and how do you manage to dissect the brain without 
injuring the individual?" 

"You don't understand; we only dissect him after he is dead." 

"But then he has ceased to act." 

"Yes." 

"But you perhaps dissect the different types of men and observe 
the different nerve-end connections?" 

"Well, no. We usually dissect paupers and the unclaimed bodies 
of human derelicts." 

"Are their synapses the same as those of gifted and successful 
people?" 

"I don't know." 

"Don't know! See here, why don't you know?" 

"Well, the synapses are not really facts of observation." 

"Do you mean to say," said he, "that you build your science on 
hypothetical assumptions? Now in Mars, our psychology is far more 
advanced. We begin by " 

Unfortunately, just then I awoke. 



The study of human nature goes back very far in the history of 
thought and has taken so many false leads that new starts have to 
be undertaken whenever the old efforts have proven misleading. The 
older structures have to be wrecked and new buildings planned in 
their stead. At the present time the wrecking crew is very busy and 
very happy. The young men who are demolishing the instinct 
hypothesis remind one of the Irish Catholic laborer who was rejoicing 
at the best job in the world: "Tearing down a Methodist church and 
getting paid for it." The instincts must go. 

The instinct hypothesis arose out of the effort to apply the con- 
cept of evolution to the facts of human behavior, and it is the doc- 
trine of evolution that must next be overthrown as a starting-point 
for social psychology. I do not mean that any concession is to be 
made to Mr. Bryan and his fundamentalism, but I do mean that, 
whether applied to individual behavior or to social change, the con- 
cept of evolution has been not only sterile of results but has been 



40 TEE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

positively misleading and has operated to obscure the real facts. He 
who will read the two books of W. H. R. Rivers, the monograph on 
the Todas and his masterpiece on the Melanesians cannot hope to 
find a more eloquent commentary on the new light and added insight 
that comes when the concept evolution has been discarded and the 
notion of history and contacts substituted. 

All this is disconcerting to some of our number, but the rejection 
of McDougallism was made necessary in most cases on account of 
the earnest effort to apply the doctrine. And the same thing can be 
said about the doctrine of the evolution of society, with its separate 
stages and successive ages. We accepted it as good and tried to 
work it, but it would not work, so we had to abandon it. 

Whence came the facts referred to? Speaking generally, they 
came from a widened view of space and a lengthened view of time 
in short, from ethnology and from history. He who will go round 
the planet, considering one by one the peoples and the cultural or- 
ganizations, and he who will consider the mere tithe that our histo- 
rians and archaeologists have furnished to us of the weary road along 
which the race has traveled, often to its doom he, I say, who will 
consider these facts will find the conceptions of instincts and evolu- 
tion incumbrances and not helps. 

Let us then consider evolutionism and instinctism dead and 
buried (or perhaps buried alive). Is the ground clear? Well, hardly. 
Physiological psychology and animal psychology have descended 
upon us in the form of behaviorism, and social psychology is to be 
written in terms of reflexes. The wrecking crew will have a harder 
task just here, for it is harder to wreck a new building than an old 
one. Moreover, it is not yet finished, and the wreckers will have to 
contend with the builders, and yet it must be done. If it is not done 
we shall not ever get a science of human nature. Now human nature 
is different from animal nature, and the neurological approach not 
only has no technique for distinguishing human nature from the 
nature of the lower animals, but tends to deny that there is any 
essential difference. But we are interested in just that difference and 
are determined to study it as it is. 

Some years ago it was a common practice to assume a primitive 



THE SUBJECTIVE ASPECT OF CULTURE 41 

man, a hairy cave man who went abroad alone, hunting and killing 
in solitary predaciousness, doing his wooing with a baseball bat and 
dragging his unconscious bride to his lair. The picture strikes us 
now as a bit inaccurate. We think of the cave man as probably hav- 
ing been born, and presumably born young, with a mother and a 
period of infancy. We think of his bride as probably not always and 
everywhere reluctant to be married. We feel superior to the men 
who wrote about the cave man in the traditional way, and yet some 
of our assumptions are open to the suspicion of an analogous error, 
for much of current writing takes the form of an inquiry into what 
the individual is, apart from his social influences. It would be as diffi- 
cult to talk about what brandy is apart from its alcoholic content. 
It would not be brandy. What the individual would be apart from 
his environment is forever impossible to state. 

It is physiological psychology and neurological psychology which 
is so largely responsible for the misleading statement of the problem 
of heredity and environment. 

Heredity and environment are not forces operating upon a pas- 
sive individual. They are not forces that compete within an individ- 
ual. They are not forces at all. Heredity and environment are tools 
of analysis, necessary and useful abstractions like weight, color, 
impenetrability. Chromosomes are concepts which the biologists 
have developed, increasingly fruitful in guiding their researches, 
but it cannot be too clearly understood that the social psychologist 
must do his work in interpreting the phenomena in his field without 
a microscope. President Coolidge opposes a competitive naval pro- 
gram, while Secretary Wilbur actively and aggressively insists that 
we should spend one hundred million dollars a year for the next 
twenty years to make our battleships many and deadly. Far be it 
from me to deny the existence of chromosomes in Secretary Wilbur 
and President Coolidge; but if I am gracious enough to admit the 
existence of their chromosomes, I shall be willing to do so only on 
condition that our biological psychologists shall speak of their 
chromosomes subsequent to, and not before, a microscopic examina- 
tion of them. 

It is environment when we cannot account for a divergence by 



42 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

an appeal to heredity. It is heredity when we cannot account for it 
by reference to the environment. It is neither heredity nor environ- 
ment when we do not have to account for it at all. 

The study of human personality has witnessed more than one 
particular divergence from sound method. Descartes was not an 
environmentalist. At least the central and most important ideas 
were to him inborn. Men gave up this notion because the divergence 
of innate ideas brought on too rich a variety for this explanation to 
satisfy. John Stuart Mill, on the other hand, was an environmen- 
talist. To him ideas came into the mind from without, being formed 
from the sensations that the environment provided. Men gave up 
this notion because of the stubbornness of youth against the efforts 
of their schoolmasters. McDougall is again an instinctivist a mili- 
tant apostle of heredity: although ideas are not inherited, yet in- 
stincts are native. The British Empire is to him the result of the 
instinctive curiosity which is so highly developed in the insular Nor- 
dics, while African slavery is the consequence of an exaggerated in- 
stinct of submission among those who have kinky hair. Men are 
giving this up because the changing trends which history reveals 
and the divergent customs which ethnology discovers have made it 
unworkable. Barnard is again an environmentalist; habits formed 
under the pressure of the environment determine the nature and 
development of personality. 

It seems that we are ready for another stage in our thinking on 
this problem. Like many other such problems, we shall not settle 
it; we shall outgrow it. There are situations in which it is of no 
value to regard either heredity or environment. Some facts of lifp 
can only be explained as due to heredity; others must refer to social 
influence. But the separation of heredity and environment can only 
be done by an act of abstraction. They never occur separately, nor 
do they ever occur together. They do not occur at all save where the 
investigator analyzes the concept and selects a single aspect to aid 
him in his puzzle. 

There is at the present time a lively but useless controversy as 
to the nature of social psychology. Social psychology is the study 
of group behavior, cries one. Nay, says another, it is the study of 
the individual modified by social influence. Who is right? Neither. 



THE SUBJECTIVE ASPECT OF CULTURE 43 

The problem belongs not in psychology but in lexicography. In the 
Oxford dictionary there are fourteen pages devoted to the definition 
of the preposition "of." Social psychology can perhaps be defined 
in a shorter space, but the facts are that men are studying groups 
and men are studying individuals, and if they can speak a language 
sufficiently common to enable them to communicate, it should not 
appear quixotic to hope that some day they may be of service to 
each other. One would have thought that Professor Cooley had 
settled this question for us long ago. If I read him aright the differ- 
ence between the individual and society does not always exist, but 
it may exist whenever a problem of their relationship appears and 
when it appears the individual is seen to be one aspect of the whole. 

They tell of a Russian Jew who came to the hospital for an 
operation on his face. He was told that it would be necessary to 
shave his beard off, a statement which produced in him the greatest 
distress. He appeared on the appointed day, his face perfectly 
smooth, having pulled every hair out by the roots. This was clearly 
the act of an individual. Here were obviously involved chromo- 
somes and nerves and reflexes. But his behavior was the subject 
aspect of an ancient culture. The rabbis of the Middle Ages pulled 
their hair out. 

McKay, a Negro poet, wrote a few years ago a defiant poem 
which millions of Negroes have read and memorized, called, "If 
We Must Die." He belonged to the race which was assumed to 
have an exaggerated instinct of submission. But there is no sub- 
mission here, and the point is that in one sense McKay did not write 
if. His race wrote it. He was the concentrated point with the wit 
to give expression to what millions were feeling before he wrote 
and felt more strongly when he wrote it. 

John Dewey once said facetiously of the Germans: "Other na- 
tions are proud of their great men; but the Germans are proud of 
themselves for producing Luther and Goethe." But in one sense the 
Germans are right. They did produce their great men, and they 
have a right to be proud of them and we of ours. 

I am seeking in this paper a formula of reconciliation. To me the 
concept of a group mind or over-soul is untenable and emotionally 
distasteful, and yet groups exist. Groups are always composed of 



44 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

members, and phenomena go on in terms that can be described, if 
one needs to do so, from the standpoint of individual psychology. 
Nevertheless, groups exist in the sense that we can identify them, 
study them, get information about them and learn to handle them. 
This is the only helpful sense in which anything may be said to exist. 
From the standpoint of our problem, the institutions of society, our 
customs, our language, our art, morals, religion, and social organ- 
ization are the objective phenomena about which facts can be gath- 
ered and generalizations made and tested. 

But in all these groups there are members, and in each of these 
members the objective aspects of culture have a corresponding sub- 
jective side, and the subjective aspect of culture is one way in which 
the object-matter of social psychology might be defined. 

A lieutenant was drilling his men. After each command he said 
in a low voice, "You too/' To his colonel, demanding an explana- 
tion, he replied: "I myself came up from the ranks, and know what 
those men are thinking. Whenever I give a command, every one of 
those men says in his heart, 'You go to hell.'" The lieutenant was a 
social psychologist. He was interested in an aspect of culture which 
is not easily accessible to strictly behavioristic methods. 

Social psychologists should rejoice at the lively interest in the 
field, and at the activity which characterizes the students of the 
subject. But we are still too disconnected in our points of view, 
and can hardly be said to have a universe of discourse. I wonder if 
we all read each others' books! It matters little what definition you 
give to social psychology, but it does matter much what method 
and standpoint we take. 

Social psychology is defined as study of groups. It is just that 
as some men pursue it. Social psychology is defined as the study of 
individuals, and some are doing that. But the important matter to 
be grasped is that both are partial, and each a different aspect of 
the whole. 

Personality is the subjective aspect of culture and the problem is 
not how personality is modified by culture, but how personality 
results inside the process of culture. 

There is doubtless a crowd fallacy and yet groups exist. They 
exist in the same sense that storms exist, or waterfalls, or stampedes. 
Groups exist in the sense that we can deal with them, study them, 



THE SUBJECTIVE ASPECT OF CULTURE 45 

get information about them, and learn about life by studying them. 

And persons exist, and in the same way, and in no other way. 
For existence can only be apprehended by us as a function. 

Consider language. Language has its subjective aspect and facts 
about language, which can be apprehended in no other way. But 
language has its objective aspect, and there are facts that can be 
obtained only by an objective consideration. No one understands 
the subjective aspect of language without some conception of the 
other, and vice versa. 

Why do we study social psychology? There is the mere urge of 
curiosity for some, but I freely confess that my own motivation is 
the hope that we shall get some of the principles of behavior so well 
formulated that we shall be enabled to control our life better. War, 
crime, poverty, vice, delinquency, and inefficiency these are the 
drives that make us eager to perfect our method and cultivate our 
field. 

And what should our method be? I am convinced that the study 
of the concrete facts of observation, experience, including intro- 
spective experience the taking into account of ethnological dif- 
ferences and cultural and historical changes these will be far more 
fruitful than any other, at least for the present. In short, our method 
must be frankly analytical, and we must be content to seek patiently 
the facts and to build on surer foundations. 

The conception of social psychology as the study of the subjec- 
tive aspect of culture, if taken wholeheartedly, will mean more than 
a mere recognition of individual and social facts. Many writers can 
repeat Cooley's phrase about society and the individual being dif- 
ferent aspects of the same phenomenon without either grasping the 
real significance of the statement or adding anything to their own 
competence in investigation. The conception of personality as sub- 
jective culture will seem to lead to very real changes of stress and 
emphasis, among which we may venture to include the following: 

1. Abandonment of the neurological and physiological approach, 
since anatomy and physiology may be assumed to be constant in any 
given series of cultural changes. 

2. Abandonment of anecdotes of animals as material, since ani- 
mals have no culture. 

3. Abandonment of hypothetical elemental infantile behavior as 



46 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

significant, and a renewed enthusiasm for careful study of children. 
Children have culture; infants are mere animals. 

4. Renewal of emphasis on imagination, since images and sym- 
bols are the essential material for the formation of social attitudes, 
and since images are essential components of wishes. As Cooley 
says, we must imagine imaginations. 

5. Increased emphasis on, and study of, emotional behavior, and 
the location of the central problem in those crises where old habits 
break up and new objects and new attitudes are formed. 

6. A renewed emphasis on communication and gesture, and the 
development for objectively studying these, since culture results 
from interaction and is transmitted in the interpenetration of at- 
titudes and gestures. 

These will, it seems certain, make more significant certain types 
of data which have recently been relegated to a subordinate place, 
but which ought to be more fruitful than ever. The chief of these 
are: ethnological facts, historical facts and changes, culture contacts, 
and individual life histories. 



A BIOLOGICAL VIEW OF RACE MIXTURE 

L. C. DUNN 

Storrs Agricultural Experiment Station 



ABSTRACT 

A biological mew of race mixture. After a brief survey of certain general principles 
of inheritance, a few cases of race mixture are discussed. Reasons for the paucity of 
admissible data are noted, and a few tentative conclusions drawn. Popular assumptions 
[>f hybrid inferiority are shown to lack support. Biological evidence indicates that 
neither inbreeding nor outbreeding have uniform effects, and that each case of crossing 
may have to be considered as a special problem. 



BIOLOGICAL FUNDAMENTALS 

Before examining the evidence on race mixture we should pre- 
pare ourselves by recalling the present status of the general prin- 
ciples of inheritance in animals. I think we may say with safety that 
but one method of inheritance has been established with certainty 
in the higher animals. This is the method first outlined by Mendel. 
The essence of the Mendelian discovery consists in the hypothesis 
that the heritable characters which differentiate individuals act as 
though they were influenced by separate and discrete units which 
segregate sharply from one another during the formation of the re- 
productive cells, and retain their identity throughout the processes 
of inheritance and development. The organism, according to the 
modern Mendelian view, is a mosaic of separable traits resulting 
from the interaction of a vast number of inherited units with each 
other and with the environment. The remarkable constancy of these 
units, and the manner in which they are distributed among the 
reproductive cells and the progeny in accordance with orderly sta- 
tistical laws is now common knowledge. It has become so common, 
Indeed, as to have invaded the popular literature of such Streitfragen 
as racial purity, immigration, race antagonism, etc. 

It is true that the Mendelian units of inheritance do appear to 
be relatively permanent and unchangeable, and that they are dis- 
tributed by fixed mathematical laws. But, as Jennings has recently 

47 



48 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

insisted, the characters of organisms are not completely determined 
or fixed by these Mendelian units. It has become more and more 
apparent that these units represent only a certain type of reaction 
to a given set of conditions. Under different conditions the identical 
hereditary constitution may produce a different set of characters. 
Moreover, these inherited units or genes interact not only with the 
environment but with each other. Each character of the organism 
is the resultant of the activity of many units, and likewise each unit 
probably affects not only one but many parts or characters of the 
animal body. This more modern view of heredity, which is in accord 
with the most recent biological research, is more flexible, less dog- 
matic and lends less countenance to the fatalistic philosophy which 
either sponsored or was invoked by hasty conclusions from a partial 
knowledge of Mendelian inheritance. 

In any discussion into which inheritance enters as a determining 
factor, as it undoubtedly does in the biological characters of races 
or hybrids, it is important to remember that genetics has contributed 
little to the settlement of the old question of "heredity or environ- 
ments" nor has it answered the question whether acquired characters 
are inherited. The most that can be said at present is that the first 
question is no longer "heredity or environment," but "heredity and 
environment how do they interact?" While concerning the second 
it is apparent that fundamental changes (mutations) in the heredi- 
tary material occur very rarely, if at all, under the direct influence 
of environmental stimuli. All attempts to generalize the unsatis- 
factory evidence on the acquired-characters dispute have collapsed, 
although not all of them in so lamentable a fashion as the recent 
efforts of Kammerer. 

The most important contributions of genetics consist in the 
demonstration that inheritance is particulate, and in the description 
of the behavior of the particles or genes under given conditions. The 
evidence is now strongly in favor of the hypothesis that the genes 
are discrete material entities located in the chromosomes, and that 
their distribution among the progeny is conditioned by the behavior 
of the chromosomes and their parts during the processes of matura- 
tion and fertilization. Usually in matings between any two animals 
such as man, a large number of hereditary differences mental, mor- 



A BIOLOGICAL VIEW OF RACE MIXTURE 49 

phological, and physiological are involved, and many of these seg- 
regate from one another at random according to the original Men- 
delian view. 

The widespread occurrence and regularity of this process by 
which each visible character of the organism appeared to be inher- 
ited independently of every other at first led to rather sweeping but 
erroneous generalizations, and it is from this era that many of the 
popularized notions of Mendelian heredity date. For it was soon 
discovered that complete independence of traits in inheritance is 
only a special condition to be encountered by chance in certain traits 
and in certain animals or plants. Two correlating agencies are known 
by which several characters may be inherited together, or as a block. 
The first of these is the occurrence, in close proximity in the repro- 
ductive material, of the genes or units of the several associated 
traits, so that they tend to be transmitted to the progeny more often 
together than separately. The second is the occurrence in the animal 
body, at least of various centers of correlation, such as the ductless 
glands which influence through such media as the circulation, diverse 
parts and characters of the body and mind. A hereditary variation 
in one of these structures is known to have a general effect, and ex- 
presses itself in many ways, so that all changes due to this source 
appear to be inherited together rather than separately. This, of 
course, is only a special case of a broader truth: that each hereditary 
unit has not merely a specific, but a general effect as well on many 
characters of the organism. This is an especially important concept 
at this time when the endocrines are invoked to explain so many of 
the differences between men, and especially between races. Of the 
actual inheritance of endocrine variations, aside from a few patho- 
logical defects, almost nothing is known. 

It is unnecessary to discuss at greater length the details and 
complexities of modern genetic theory. These add little to the gen- 
eral conception of unit inheritance, the interaction, association, and 
manifold effects of the inherited units, and the reciprocal interplay 
between heredity and environment as outlined above. The data 
on human inheritance and race mixture are as yet too meager in 
amount to require more than the elementary general ideas of 
genetics. 



50 TEE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

THE DATA OF RACE MIXTURE 

The first important observation to make concerning the biolog- 
ical data of race mixture is that they are both few and fragmentary. 
There is no dearth of scattered observations, but most of them can- 
not be admitted because of the non-critical nature of the evidence. 
The rise of physical anthropology with its insistence on exact meas- 
urements statistically analyzed, and the new knowledge of inherit- 
ance with its present emphasis on specific traits rather than on the 
body as a whole have quite altered the nature of the evidence re- 
quired. The data on which the conclusions of the last century rested 
no longer satisfy the cautious and impartial student of today. 

The nature of the human material itself also explains in part the 
paucity of material. The present human race is a biological unit. 
All crosses are fertile and result in fertile hybrids. Moreover, man 
is an anciently domesticated and a widely distributed species. These 
conditions favor intercrossing between different groups. On the 
other side and working toward dose breeding are the circumstances 
of geographical isolation and the feeling of race prejudice. Each of 
these latter is in the life of a species of relatively temporary duration, 
although they may be operative for sufficiently long periods to pro- 
tect groups from outcrossing, and thus enable variability within the 
group to be reduced and group peculiarities to develop and become 
relatively fixed. The absence of sterility is, however, a continuing 
and permanent aid to crossing, and the movements of peoples sooner 
or later provide the opportunity. That crossbreeding has taken place 
from the earliest times is evident to the biologist who has come to 
ascribe to the inherently variable types a history of crossbreeding, 
and to the pure and less variable types a degree of antecedent in- 
breeding. And in turning from animal to human material he is at 
once impressed with the enormous variability in evidence. In the 
absence of anything resembling pure types, the investigator must 
work in a complex and fluid medium which contains no datum points 
or bench marks, conditions which impede the progress of racial in- 
vestigations. 

The evidence on which we may rely for tentative answers to our 
purely biological questions must also be as little complicated as 
possible by the complex social environment which results when hy- 



A BIOLOGICAL VIEW OF RACE MIXTURE 51 

brid types live in continuing contact with the parent types, particu- 
larly if these happen to be of the so-called civilized type. The type 
of evidence fulfilling these requirements is such as that which Fischer 
obtained in the interesting hybrid community which he investigated 
in South Africa. Some material has been obtained from Hawaii, 
where hybrids between the chief racial groups of the world are found 
in a single environment, and most recently Shapiro has obtained 
valuable observations on the hybrid population of Norfolk Island 
in the Pacific, and has very kindly given me access to parts of his 
data. In addition to the above purely racial evidence, there are 
numerous systematic observations on the inheritance of specific 
human traits, some of which throw light on racial inheritance. As 
an example of the type of evidence which is not entirely acceptable, 
may be mentioned the observations of Mjoen as presented in a recent 
paper at the Second International Congress of Eugenics. The bio- 
logical conclusions of Mjoen have been severely criticized by Castle, 
but his evidence from human hybrids appears to partake of the 
nature of the experimental evidence from animal hybrids. 

The evidence from Fischer's study of Boer-Hottentot hybrids 
has been published for some years and is well known. The evidence 
from Hawaii relating to Hawaiian- Chinese hybrids has been partially 
recorded, and the other observations on race mixture between 
Hawaiians and Europeans are in process of publication. Perhaps 
the most interesting material of all, that of Shapiro on the Tahitian- 
English hybrids of Norfolk Island, is in process of analysis, and is 
not yet available. The latter case has been known for many years 
as an exceptional opportunity for the study of race mixture uncom- 
plicated by contact of the hybrids with parent stocks. A century 
and a half ago ten English sailors who had mutinied from the ship 
"Bounty," together with six Tahitian men and twelve Tahitian 
women, escaped from Tahiti to the tiny island of Pitcairn in the 
mid-Pacific, and there founded the hybrid race which now occupies 
both Pitcairn and Norfolk Islands. 

The data on the physical characteristics of these diverse types 
of hybrids are in substantial agreement. When considered together 
with the results of studies in human inheritance they afford partial 
answers to some of our general questions. 



52 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

In the first place, they leave little doubt that human traits, even 
those which differentiate races, are inherited in accordance with the 
Mendelian conception of inheritance. This conclusion rests in part 
on the general results of studies in human inheritance, and to a lesser 
extent on the data from racial crosses where the complex and quan- 
titative character of most of the differences has prevented an exact 
analysis. Such traits as head-form and bodily proportions differ 
among the races of the world on a quantitative scale with overlap- 
ping variability. The inheritance of such traits in man, as in animals 
and plants, is apparently complex. Usually the hybrids are inter- 
mediate in any such character between the parental conditions, and 
increased variability and combinations in one individual of traits 
from both parental races are the only evidence that these traits rest 
on a genetic basis similar to that which obtains in other animals. 
This evidence, however, is usually accepted by geneticists as an 
indication that such traits are influenced by many factors which 
differ in number rather than in kind from those which affect simpler, 
qualitative traits. 

The intermediate nature of the hybrids indicates that such char- 
acters do not usually show dominance. This is not an unfailing cri- 
terion of Mendelian inheritance, since in most animal crosses the 
complete disappearance or non-expression of one of the contrasted 
parental traits in the hybrid offspring can no longer be regarded as 
the rule. 

Few racial traits show dominance, but when it does happen that 
some prominent racial character of a given race does appear in the 
majority of its hybrid progeny it is sometimes concluded that this 
indicates a greater general prepotency on the part of that race, 
which means, I suppose, a greater ability of this race to stamp its 
characters on the progeny. Thus the negro skin-color and the Mon- 
golian type of eyelid appear to be partially dominant; that is, they 
are expressed in the hybrid condition. But this means only that 
traits which because of their uniformity within the parent race have 
come by long experience to be accepted as the hall mark of the race 
do happen to show dominance. Other elements in the racial com- 
plex do not show dominance, as, for example, the head-shape of the 
Chinese, which appears to be partially recessive to the broader head- 



A BIOLOGICAL VIEW OF RACE MIXTURE 53 

form of the Hawaiians. It may happen that one of the races par- 
ticipating in the cross possesses more visible or measurable dominant 
traits than the other, as, for example, the South Chinese as compared 
with the Hawaiians; but the behavior of these traits in inheritance 
shows that the chance association of dominance with noticeable 
traits is sufficient without invoking a mysterious racial prepotency. 

That segregation and recombination of parental characters does 
take place is, however, unquestionable even in the meager data at 
hand. Diverse combinations occur as early as the second generation 
and one finds, for example, in the second generation from a cross of 
Hawaiian by Chinese, individuals with the stature of the Hawaiian 
parent, the hair-form of the Chinese, the head-form of the Hawaiian, 
the peculiar Mongoloid eye of the Chinese, etc. There is in the data 
from Hottentot-Boer, Negro-white, Hawaiian-white, Hawaiian- 
Chinese, or Tahitian-white crosses as observed by Shapiro on Nor- 
folk Island no evidence of permanent blends of racial traits. An 
intermediate condition of certain traits may become established if 
assortative mating of special types takes place, but the rule is that 
the single characters tend to segregate from the cross in different 
combinations in later generations. 

This segregation is the basis of the universally observable in- 
crease in variability of hybrid populations in generations beyond the 
first. The variation curves of the characters of Fischer's Boer-Hot- 
tentot hybrids furnish good examples of this fact. It is unquestion- 
ably true that variability is increased by crossing. 

Do new traits either advantageous or disadvantageous arise as 
a result of crossing? The evidence does not establish that they do. 
But there is no question that new and unique combinations of traits 
arise. Superficially this would appear to produce the same result as 
the genesis of entirely new traits. But its evolutionary significance 
is quite different, for combinations are limited by the number of 
single traits, and are found to resolve again into their elements by 
independent inheritance; while a new trait, if heritable and not dis- 
advantageous, increases by a calculable amount the number of po- 
tential combinations. In animal experimentation there is no evi- 
dence that new inherited units arise any oftener under crossbreeding 
than under any other system of mating. 



54 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

Are human hybrids more vigorous or less than the parent types? 
Are they under any biological handicaps such as infertility? Are the 
new combinations of characters in hybrids disharmonious or incom- 
patible? Dogmatic answers can certainly not be given from the hu- 
man data. The Boer-Hottentot hybrids and the Norfolk population 
are certainly at least the physical equals of either parent race. In 
Hawaii the physical measurements of hybrids, while they do not in- 
dicate a pronounced hybrid vigor, show that the hybrids are not 
inferior. And in the opinion of more than one observer some of the 
hybrid groups, e.g., the Hawaiian-Chinese, represent a physical im- 
provement of the parent types. 

Disharmonic types undoubtedly do exist among hybrids, but 
only in the sense that combinations of traits occur which are not 
normal or frequent in purer types. As far as can be ascertained from 
physical measurements these new combinations are not injurious, 
and no derogatory significance need be attached to disharmony. It 
is a normal occurrence after crossing. 

With regard to fecundity, the evidence is fragmentary and diffi- 
cult of interpretation. There is a lack of good biological evidence on 
human fecundity in general, in the absence of which it is impossible 
to say how much of the often-noted differences in this respect be- 
tween races and their hybrids rest on a biological basis and how much 
is due to economic and social causes. The birth-rate itself is an ex- 
pression of the interaction of these several factors and of another 
item by no means negligible, i.e., the amount of mortality among the 
unborn offspring. The last may be somewhat lower in mixed than 
in pure matings. Little, for example, found a greater proportion of 
still births from intraracial or intranational matings than from mat- 
ings of different race or nationality. There is some evidence from 
animals which shows that crossing tends to prevent the expression 
of factors having an adverse effect on embryonic development, while 
pure matings allow greater scope for the combination and expression 
of such factors. In regard to fecundity itself, or the number of poten- 
tial offspring produced in the absence of social and economic checks, 
we can draw no conclusions. A lowered gross fecundity has not been 
established for cross-matings, and the hybrid groups considered are 
experiencing no difficulty from biological causes in reproducing and 
increasing in number. 



A BIOLOGICAL VIEW OF RACE MIXTURE 55 

A survey of the few general characteristics of racial hybrids 
which have been mentioned does not lead to the conclusion that 
mixed races are in any biological sense inferior to purer types. The 
kinds of deterioration which are often alleged to follow race-crossing 
may be shown to follow many intraracial matings, and to be due to 
social and environmental factors. One can see scant comfort for 
any hypotheses of racial deterioration based on hybrid inferiority as 
a biological postulate. On the other hand, the extreme examples 
cited, such as Fischer's Rehoboth hybrids and the hybrid groups of 
Hawaii and Norfolk, show that mixed races even of recent origin 
may be biologically quite as successful as unmixed types which have 
undergone a longer period of selection in a given environment. 

It is quite possible, however, that general statements concerning 
the effect of race mixture are quite beside the point, and that the 
biological characters of races or hybrids are determined by factors 
which are in a measure independent of the conditions of purity or 
mixture. Such a view has become more and more prevalent among 
biologists as a result of the work of the last two decades on the effects 
of different systems of mating in animals and plants. Continued 
close breeding is now known to lead generally to reduced variability, 
while in some cases vigor and stamina decline, and in others is main- 
tained. Crossing results generally in increased variability, and fre- 
quently in increased vigor. These general results have been recon- 
ciled and brought into line with current genetic theory by the 
assumption that both kinds of effects are due to the dominance, 
segregation, and recombination of separable factors. The system of 
mating, whether close or crossbreeding, produces its results only by 
virtue of its effect on the distribution of the specific inherited units, 
which assume the role of first importance. According to this view, 
the specific inheritance received is of greater effect than the inbred 
or crossbred nature of the individual or species. If this is correct, 
and the bulk of the evidence is in its favor, then the effect of race 
mixture will depend in each case on the specific inherited units re- 
ceived from each parent race, and it is to be expected that different 
results will be obtained from different crosses. Crossing between 
races becomes then a special case of crossing between individuals, in 
which it has already been established that the inherent nature of the 
offspring depends on the genetic constitution of the parents. The 



56 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

peculiarity of the special case consists in the greater number of in- 
herited differences involved in racial crosses rather than in the kind 
of differences. These differences in number only make the biological 
study of race mixture more complex; they do not set it apart as a 
problem to be studied and interpreted with a new or peculiar set of 
symbols or hypotheses. The large problem of race mixture will be- 
come progressively broken up into single problems which will deal 
with each case of race mixture as a special case. 

The present evidence in regard to mental differences between 
races leads to the same conclusions as that in regard to physical 
traits, that is, that races are unlike, and probably innately so. There 
is no good evidence on the behavior of these differences following 
race mixture, but there is no reason to expect that, if they are as 
real as the physical differences, they should behave differently. 

All that has gone before is an implicit plea for an objective view 
of racial problems. This can only be gained through the recognition 
of the reality and of the biological and social significance of race. 
Striking racial differences exist, which are not abolished, but com- 
bine and endure through cross-matings. These diverse combinations 
and the variability which results may be one condition of evolution- 
ary progress in man as in the lower animals and plants. It may be 
suggested that in a complex civilization which rests on division of 
labor, variability is even more essential than in more primitive soci- 
eties. Real racial differences may then be the raw materials needed 
for an enlarging society. 

DISCUSSION 



F. H. HANKINS 

Smith College 



Some points in Professor Dunn's paper will bear reiteration and others ex- 
pansion. In the first place, it cannot be too often repeated nowadays that pure 
races, in the sense in which the experimental biologist or animal breeder uses 
the term, do not exist in the human stock, though some groups are, obviously, 
purer than others. All the great groups represented by the terms white, black, 
or yellow, are highly complex from the standpoint of genetic composition. The 
same is true of such ill-defined racial groups as the Nordics or the Anglo-Saxons. 

Those who, therefore, claim that the so-called Nordic race may be likened 



DISCUSSION 57 

to a high-bred strain of domestic animals are drawing heavily on their will to 
believe. In such animal strains there has been a great deal of selective breeding, 
whereas in the populations among which the Nordic element has existed for 
many thousands of years there has prevailed an approximation to panmixia. 
In his study of the Old Americans, who may be taken as representative of the 
so-called Anglo-Saxon race, Hrdiicka found that mixed types greatly predom- 
inate, and that pure brunets are more frequent than pure blonds. It is safe to 
say that all the populations of civilized countries are not only hybrid, but 
multi-split hybrids. 

Secondly, not the least interesting disclosure of recent genetic experimenta- 
tion is the fact that the same inherent constitution may react differently to 
different environments. In technical terms, the same genotype, or inherent 
constitution, may give rise to different pheno types, or actual organic forms, 
under different environmental conditions. Such changes involve no change in 
genotypic or idiotypic nature or possibilities and are not to be confused with 
mutations. But it would be a mistake to imply that an organism as resistant 
as the human is likely to show measurable differences in phenotypic responses 
to ordinary variations in environment. Boas' famous investigation of changes 
in the head-form of immigrants cannot be accepted unequivocally, and even if 
accepted shows such differences to be small. 

Another point that will bear re-emphasis is the tendency toward blending 
in the first hybrid generation and toward segregation of traits in succeeding 
generations, but with little evidence of clear-cut or complete dominance. This 
matter is greatly complicated by the fact that the unit characters of the popular 
textbooks are much less simple on the human level than in four-o 'clocks. Some 
lists of such characters have been given by Davenport, Conklin, and Holmes, 
but they are certainly not very reliable. The complicating factor appears often 
to be of a quantitative sort, as illustrated by degrees of pigmentation. This makes 
possible a retention of a blended condition even after some segregation has 
occurred. White children from mulatto parents are extremely rare. Neverthe- 
less, segregation occurs in complete form for specific traits and, together with 
assortative mating, tends to preserve the superficial aspects of the original 
parental types. 

It was this fact that led many of the racial determinists of a generation ago 
to the conclusion that certain combinations of traits were stable and tended to 
restore themselves amid a mixed population, while other combinations were 
unstable and hence tended to be eliminated. A full determination of the matter 
would require a measure of the extent of dominance and of the influence of 
assortative mating. But even when these are given all possible weight the older 
views appear out of joint with the facts. Thus, when certain blond traits, say 
blond features, segregate out from a blond-brunet hybrid stock, there is no 
ground for supposing that other traits originally associated with blond hair and 
eyes in one of the parent stocks will also segregate out. The genes segregate in 
independent fashion, except for certain closely related traits. One must con- 



58 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

elude that in such populations as those of Western Europe and the United 
States there is no general tendency to revert to the original stocks, but that an 
endless diversity of mozaic combinations of the original unit elements occur, 
like the endless variety of an infinite kaleidoscope. 

Here a word may be said about the so-called disharmonic combinations. 
Because of them numerous writers warn against racial crossings. In writers of 
the Lapouge school the disharmony consists for the most part in the unusualness 
of the combination. The usual is considered the normal. But this does not 
warrant the conclusion that the unusual is abnormal in any derogatory sense. 
Professor Castle concludes from genetic experiments against the whole doctrine. 
In any case broad generalizations, as Professor Dunn states, must give way to 
a consideration of individual cases. 

Many other points might be discussed; we limit ourselves to the question of 
variability. Hybrids show a wider variability because they permit a reshuffling 
of the genetic elements. The Gobineau-Grant school of racial determinists have 
cultivated the dogma that civilizations die because the pure race that creates 
them is mongrelized and its talent polluted by race crossing. The opposite argu- 
ment is more plausible. Civilizations arise only in areas of race mixture and 
only in such areas long after the mixing has begun. So far as biological factors 
play a part in this result they do so because race mixture produces an increased 
diversity of human talents. A single pure-bred race cannot produce geniuses of 
the highest order in all the highly diversified lines of human achievement which 
are necessary for a complex and colorful civilization. Such a distinctive racial 
type would of necessity have the limitations of its own special gifts. In other 
words, a population possesses a richer genetic basis than a race. 



THE HYBRID AS A SOCIOLOGICAL TYPE 



E. B. REUTER 
The University of Iowa 



ABSTRACT 

The hybrid as a sociological type. There are no ethnically unmixed groups in the 
modern world, and all persons are, in consequence, hybrid. These hybrids, being for the 
most part the offspring of parents of nearly related racial types, are not markedly dif- 
ferent in appearance from individuals of the parent races and are not sociologically sig- 
nificant. But in some cases the hybrids are the offspring of individuals of physically 
divergent racial groups. In such cases the hybrids are in appearance unlike the members 
of either parent race and are unable to pass as members of either the one race or the 
other. The physical appearance thus determines a social type by determining the social 
status of the hybrids in the biracial situation. In all cases these hybrid groups resulting 
from the amalgamation of physically divergent races are superior in social position and 
in intellectual achievement to one racial element of their ancestry. This superiority can- 
not be explained in biological and ancestral terms, but is readily amenable to explanation 
in terms of mobility and social contact. 



There are few questions of behavior more heavily freighted with 
emotional content than that of racial amalgamation. It touches the 
two points at which Western peoples most frequently run amuck: 
the violation of womanhood, and the integrity of the social group. 
The attitudes at each point are so completely imbedded in the under- 
lying mores as all but to preclude discussion of related topics. Also, 
there are many persons who harbor an uneasy fear that candid dis- 
cussion of racial intermixture would bring into the light facts not 
wholly flattering to a revered ancestry. There are others who regard 
any objective treatment as an attempt to challenge the validity of 
beliefs essential to the stability of the existing racial order. To cer- 
tain persons of delicate sensibilities the idea is personally distasteful; 
they are physically nauseated by the imagery of the intimacy in- 
volved. 

By simple rationalization of these emotional attitudes men derive 
opinions concerning the phenomenon that stirs their tribal fears. 
They see in the amalgamation of races a violation of the divine pur- 
pose manifest in the fact of racial dissimilarity; they see the decline 
of civilization and the recrudescence of barbarism through the con- 
tamination of the Nordic stock; they see the downfall of nations as 

59 



60 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

a result of the dilution of the political genius of peoples; or they fore- 
tell some other type of major disaster according as the individual 
run of attention determines the specific form of the rationalization. 
To forestall a train of anticipated evils men resort to external con- 
trol in its varied forms: the state denies to mixed marriages the 
protection of organized society; the church withholds her divine 
sanction; public sentiment ostracizes the participants; and unregu- 
lated mobs discipline persons violating the racial taboos of the group. 

Yet, regardless of the moral indignation aroused and of its ex- 
pression in ill-considered legislation and in acts of personal violence, 
the process of racial fusion goes on wherever individuals of divergent 
racial ancestry come into personal contact. 

While we may not assert that the fusion of races has always 
aroused the disapproval of the group, we may assert the universality 
of the process itself. The skeletal remains of fossil man leave no 
doubt that the blending of divergent stocks was in process some 
millenniums before the historic era. Examination of the various ex- 
istent backward-culture peoples shows an endless mixing of stocks 
and blending of cultures. Knowledge of historic peoples of both the 
ancient and the modern world reveals invasion and conquest, the 
fusion of cultures, and the amalgamation of the conquered with their 
conquerors as characteristic elements in the formation of states. 
Every modern marriage continues the process of hybridization of 
nearly or remotely related racial types; virtually every child is the 
hybrid offspring of a hybrid ancestry. 

The hybridization of stocks, continuous in the phenomena of 
marriage in the ethnic mosaic of modern nationalities, gives rise to 
offspring not differing in any outstanding way that would interfere 
with mobility and social contacts in a cosmopolitan society: each 
hybridized individual is a unit with a unique combination of physical 
and probably of mental traits, but sufficiently within the group 
range of variation to live an individual and unmolested life. This 
intermixture has no sociological consequences, and interests the 
social theorist only to the extent that it facilitates cultural contacts 
and contributes to the spread, acceptance, and fusion of culture 
heritages. 

But in the recent centuries, particularly since the period of the 



THE HYBRID AS A SOCIOLOGICAL TYPE 61 

discoveries, and increasingly as science through the development of 
means of transportation and communication has made possible 
world-wide trade and commercial relations, people hitherto widely 
separated have been brought into contact in ever increasing numbers. 
These ethnic contacts incident to trade, colonial, and military ac- 
tivities, have been in certain cases with peoples of distinctive physi- 
cal appearance and of retarded or divergent culture. 

When such has been the case, the sex relationships of individuals 
of the unlike races have resulted in hybrid offspring more or less 
distinctive in appearance. In America the white pioneers and settlers 
associated with and sometimes married the Indian girls. At present 
every Indian tribe contains numerous individuals of biracial ancestry 
who obviously are neither Indian nor European. In some tribes no 
individuals of pure Indian ancestry remain, and in many the half- 
breeds are more numerous than the persons of unmixed Indian an- 
cestry. Incident to the Spanish colonial policy, large numbers of 
individuals of Spanish-Indian ancestry appeared. In the Spanish 
West Indies, in Central America, and in various South American 
republics these Spanish half-breeds are elements of numerical im- 
portance in the populations. Other hybrid groups owe their origin to 
the contact of the Portuguese and Indians in South America; to the 
contact of the French and Indians in Canada; to the contact of the 
Chinese with the native races of the Philippines. In the West Indies, 
on the continent of South America, and elsewhere the Spanish came 
into contact with Negroes and left a numerous mulatto progeny. 
Other mulatto groups have appeared in South Africa and the United 
States and indeed in every situation where Negro women have en- 
countered white men. There are various half-caste groups in the 
Indian Coast cities and elsewhere in the Orient which resulted from 
the contacts incident to the trade and commercial relations between 
the East and the West and to the colonizing activities of the Euro- 
pean world. 

In each of these cases, and in various others, the mixed racial 
ancestry is plainly evident in the physical appearance of the hybrids. 
The marriage of the hybrid individuals with each other as well as 
their crossing with each of the parent races has produced an endless 
variety of types intermediate between the contrasted racial extremes. 



62 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

We are not here concerned with the physical effects of these race 
crossings with hybridization in the biological sense. But the dis- 
tinctive appearance resulting from the biological fact of intermixture 
affects the mobility and so limits the degree and conditions the type 
of social interaction. We are concerned with the sociological con- 
sequences of the intermixture of racial stocks that are sufficiently 
divergent in physical characters to give rise to an offspring readily 
distinguishable by external and ineradicable marks from the mem- 
bers of either group. 

A thing that inevitably impresses itself upon the student of com- 
parative racial phenomena is the striking similarity in social status 
and achievement of different hybrid groups. In every biracial situ- 
ation the hybrids form or tend to form a distinctive class, and to 
occupy a more or less distinctive social position. In every situation 
they have erupted a percentage of intellectually capable men far in 
excess of that furnished by the native element of their racial ancestry. 
Everywhere they have risen to positions of leadership and relative so- 
cial success in larger numbers than have the full-blooded individuals. 

A study of four or five thousand American men of Negro blood 
who have achieved in any degree that would distinguish them even 
locally from the great mediocre group shows a great preponderance 
of men of biracial ancestry. If attention be limited to the small 
group of men of Negro affiliation who have shown a type of ability 
that would be marked and exceptional in any group, the percentage 
of mixed-blood men very greatly increases. In spite of the fact that 
the Negroes of relatively unmixed Negro blood outnumber the mixed 
bloods three or four to one, they have not produced one prominent 
man to ten men of equal ability developed by the mulattoes. The 
disproportion holds in all fields of endeavor, though it is less marked 
in poetry, oratory, preaching, and other lines in which ignorance is 
not a serious handicap than in science and in scholarly and profes- 
sional pursuits where achievement is more largely conditioned by 
adequate training. The disproportion has prevailed throughout the 
history of the Negroes in America, though it is perhaps less today 
than at earlier periods. 

The facts are not essentially different in other biracial situations. 



THE HYBRID AS A SOCIOLOGICAL TYPE 63 

In Jamaica, Brazil, South Africa, and elsewhere the hybrid elements 
form an intellectual, social, and economic aristocracy that has pro- 
duced the vast majority of the men who have risen to prominence. 
There appears to be no exception to the generalization that, in bi- 
racial situations comprising two racial groups of unequal culture, 
the hybrids tend to occupy an intermediate social and cultural status 
and to produce a markedly higher percentage of men of prominence 
and leadership than does the ethnically unmixed native group. 

To understand the superior social status of hybrid groups and 
the greater accomplishment of hybrid individuals it is necessary to 
inquire concerning the facts of origin, development, and opportunity. 

A popular explanation of the superiority runs to the effect that 
hybrids, being the offspring of a so-called inferior by a so-called 
superior race, will, in accordance with the popular conception of the 
blending of biologically heritable traits, occupy an intermediate 
position between the parent races. Recently various writers have 
found experimental and statistical means for demonstrating this 
independently acquired belief. This conception of the mulatto as a 
halfway type is a phase and variation of the general doctrine of 
racial superiority which has been apparently a cherished myth of 
every human group able to observe physical or cultural differences 
between themselves and their neighbors. At present practically all 
sound scholarship in social phenomena assumes an essential equality 
in the native mentality of racial groups and, before a scholarly audi- 
ence, this sort of explanation may be dismissed without extended 
discussion. The present popular recrudescence of primitive ethno- 
centrism is to be understood as an inevitable protest of intrenched 
privilege against the democratic drift of the modern world. 

If any native superiority of the mixed-blood groups over either 
parent-race appears, the explanation must be found in terms of a 
selected ancestry. Assuming an inheritance of mental ability which 
is probably a fact, though neither the fact nor the degree nor the 
method of such inheritance has been demonstrated it must be 
shown that the ancestors of the hybrid, on either, or both, the pa- 
ternal or the maternal side, were individuals inherently superior in 
mental capacity to the average of one or both racial groups. Selec- 



64 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

tive processes must be shown to have been operative in the establish- 
ment of the group. This requires an examination of the facts of 
intermixture. 

In origin the half-breed individuals were the result, in major 
part, of extra-matrimonial relations between men of the politically 
dominant race and women of the inferior group. In America the 
white man fathered a mulatto offspring by both the free and the 
enslaved Negro woman. In Canada the French men and the Indian 
women freely intermixed. In Central and South America and in the 
West Indies the Spanish reared a numerous progeny by both the 
Negro and the Indian women. In the Philippines and elsewhere in 
the East both the Chinese traders and the Spanish conquerors used 
the native women as mothers of their half-caste offspring. On the 
African continent are numerous mixed races owing their origin to 
the contact of the Arab and other traders with women of the native 
races. The rather numerous Eurasians of the coast cities of the East 
owe their origin to the contact of Western men and Eastern women. 
In every situation the mixed-blood groups are the result of extra- 
matrimonial relations between the men of the politically and cul- 
turally dominant group and the women of the native or culturally 
retarded race. 

I am not unmindful of the fact that a marriage relation some- 
times obtains between individuals of such racial groups, nor am I 
unmindful of the fact that women of the culturally superior groups 
have from time to time married men of the culturally lower race, 
and that other such women have given birth to illegitimate half- 
breed children. But a rather trivial percentage of the hybrids of any 
country can trace their ancestry back to regular marriage unions or 
to mothers of the culture group. The almost universal rule is that 
the crossing of races took place outside the socially and legally sanc- 
tioned institution of marriage, and that the native or culturally back- 
ward race furnished the mothers. 

The inquiry thus reduces itself to a question of whether or not, 
and if at all to what extent, such a situation extra-matrimonial sex 
relations between men of a politically or culturally dominant race 
and women of a dependent race or lower culture implies a selected 
type of ancestry. 



THE HYBRID AS A SOCIOLOGICAL TYPE 65 

It is probably true that a polygamous sex system is eugenically 
desirable. It allows the men of wealth and position to choose as con- 
sorts such women and girls as please their amorous fancy, and to 
leave a larger number of offspring than other men of the society. If 
there exists any correlation between sexual attractiveness and men- 
tal ability in women, and if mental ability be a heritable trait, there 
is reason to believe that the offspring of such a selected maternal 
ancestry would be mentally superior individuals. Also, if these men 
of wealth and position are the innately superior men of the group as 
has often been asserted, though never proved the offspring should 
be innately superior persons from the paternal side of their ancestry. 
Making the necessary assumptions involved that wealth and posi- 
tion are evidence of superior native mental ability, or that native 
ability is correlated with sexual attractiveness in women, and that 
mental ability is a heritable trait it is then necessary to show only 
that the hybrids are the offspring of superior men and charming 
women. The problem thus reduces itself to the question: Were the 
hybrids fathered by the men of wealth and position, and were their 
mothers the exceptional girls of the lower-culture race? 

On this point, two or three typical cases must suffice. The half- 
breed stocks of South America are the descendants of hybrids re- 
sulting from the association of Spanish and Portuguese traders, 
priests, soldiers, functionaries, and other parasitic accompaniments 
of a colonial regime. There were doubtless among these classes men 
of average or superior native ability, but to assume that they repre- 
sented superior biological strains of the Peninsula population would 
be to make an assumption contrary to everything we know con- 
cerning such classes in all societies, and contrary to what we know 
historically in regard to these particular groups. If the hybrids of 
these countries can make any claim to a natively superior ancestry, 
it must be based upon the assumption that these white masters 
selected for their mistresses the superior members of the enslaved 
groups, with the further assumption of a positive correlation be- 
tween mental ability and the type of native female beauty that ap- 
pealed to the fancy of the Spanish colonial. 

In North America the various white peoples, chiefly from North 
and West European countries, mingled their blood with the Indian 



66 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

groups until many of the unexterminated tribes contain more in- 
dividuals of hybrid than of unmixed Indian ancestry. It may be 
true that the frontiersmen and squaw men were a selected type, but 
there is no present evidence that they represented the mental elite 
of a race. If a case is to be made for the mental superiority of the 
Indian-white hybrids over either parent race the superiority must 
come from the Indian branch of the ancestry, and to support such 
an assumption the necessary evidence appears to be lacking. 

In the crossing of races in the American Negro-white groups the 
facts are much the same. It was in part a concubinage of selected 
Negro and mulatto girls by men and boys of the socially upper 
classes. There is no question that white men of prominence and 
presumably of ability were responsible for some part of the early 
intermixture, nor is there any doubt that the slaveholding aristoc- 
racy left a more or less numerous mulatto progeny. But this does 
not represent all nor indeed the major part of the hybridization. For 
the most part the amalgamation of the races resulted from the as- 
sociation of unselected groups of both races, and did not represent 
any obvious superiority on either side of the racial ancestry. Even 
where the slaveholding groups furnished the fathers, the fact of 
superior ancestry is not demonstrated. It simply raises the further 
question as to whether and, if at all, to what extent the slave owners, 
the plantation overseers, and the slave foremen were inherently 
superior types of men. 

From the known facts in regard to racial intermixture in this and 
other countries there appears to be no sufficient evidence to justify 
a belief that the mixed-blood populations are descended from men- 
tally superior persons or groups. If there be any correlation between 
social position and native ability, the hybrid groups in their origin 
were probably below rather than above an average, their ancestry 
rather downward than upward of a social mean; there appears to be 
no adequate ground for a belief in a native superiority of the in- 
dividuals of biracial ancestry that would account for their superior 
intellectual and social status. 

The explanation of hybrid superiority must be found in social 
rather than in biological facts. It lies in the relative degree of isola- 
tion, in the differences in mobility and opportunity of the native as 



THE HYBRID AS A SOCIOLOGICAL TYPE 67 

compared with the members of mixed racial origin. For the sake of 
concreteness and brevity the discussion at this point is limited to the 
American situation which, in general outline, is typical of the differ- 
entiation process within such biracial populations. 

At the time of their first contact on American soil the Negroes 
and the whites represented the opposite poles of cultural develop- 
ment. They differed in language, customs, and habits of life; in 
moral, mental, and religious development, as well as in ethnic origin, 
historical tradition, and physical appearance. A black skin, there- 
fore, came early to signify inferior culture and only a little later be- 
came the badge of a servile condition. Between the two races there 
could be no general social equality; there was not even a possibility 
of harmonious working relations except on the basis of superiority 
and subordination. 

When individuals of mixed ancestry presently appeared there 
was manifested no disposition on the part of the whites to treat 
them as essentially different from the Negroes. In large part they 
were the offspring of a class of white men whose social, economic, 
and political status was not at first markedly superior to that of the 
Negroes; when such was not the case the bastard origin of the mulat- 
toes shocked the conventional moral sense of the community and 
militated against a community recognition of them as superior to 
the Negroes of full blood. This attitude presently found formal ex- 
pression in the legislative enactments which assigned the mulattoes 
to the status of their mothers. 

But the individual mulattoes were believed to be more capable 
than the full-blood Negroes. Consequently, occupational differen- 
tiation within the race operated to their advantage: the favored 
classes among the slaves, as the number of mulattoes increased, 
came more and more to be light-colored classes; and the trained 
mechanics and the trusted servants were drawn from these lighter- 
colored groups. Moreover, the mulattoes made a better appearance 
than the black Negroes, were less offensive in close association, and 
so gravitated to those house and personal duties which brought them 
into personal association with the master class. The plantation 
slaves and the rough laborers in the cities and the towns were largely 
black men. The division was, of course, not everywhere equally 



68 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

marked, and it was seldom a sharp and complete separation. There 
were many full-blood black men among the favored classes and 
there were mulattoes in considerable number in the lower slave 
occupations, but there was a persistent tendency toward an occupa- 
tional separation on the basis of color. Manumission further widened 
the breach that existed in bondage. The free Negro group at all 
times contained a preponderance of mulattoes; in some places it was, 
to all intent and purpose, a mulatto group. Such education of the 
Negro as existed before the Emancipation was almost entirely mulat- 
to education; it was limited to the free Negroes and to certain favored 
individuals and groups among the slaves. Throughout the period of 
slavery the rational as well as the sentimental judgments of the 
whites operated to make the mulattoes superior men and to make 
the superior groups in the Negro population mulatto groups. 

The superior achievement of the hybrids of divergent culture 
races over the native element of their racial ancestry is a historic 
fact too well established to admit of question. Its explanation ap- 
pears to rest not in the biological fact of mixed blood as such but in 
the culture contacts and personal mobility consequent upon the 
mixed ethnic origin. It is the result of a differential treatment de- 
termined by the biological fact, and is thus not an evidence of supe- 
rior capacity but a reasonable measure of superior opportunity. 



AN ANTHROPOLOGICAL VIEW OF RACE MIXTURE 



RALPH LINTON 

Field Museum of Natural History 



ABSTRACT 

An anthropological mew of race mixture. The whole question of the desirability of 
race mixture is inextricably bound up with that of racial equality, for if the white man 
is really superior to the dark-skinned races he should do everything in his power to keep 
his stock pure. We have, as yet, no conclusive evidence of racial inequality, from the 
data of physical anthropology, cultural evolution, or mental testing. A really scientific 
approach to the problems of race mixture is rendered difficult by our inability to breed 
pure strains together under test conditions. The little information we have does not 
indicate that hybrids are inferior to pure strains either mentally or physically. The 
Polynesians, for example, arc an unusually fine group. Practically all the civilization 
of which we have record have been the work of peoples who were mixed in blood, while 
pure groups are usually somewhat backward in culture. It seems certain that the 
hybrid is as capable of preserving and adding to civilization as the pure-bred individual. 
The absorption into the white population of the United States of our present Indian, 
Mongol, and Negro minorities is not likely to influence our culture unfavorably. 



American students are perhaps less fitted to deal with the prob- 
lems of racial equality and of the results of race mixture than those 
of European countries, for we find it hard to approach these ques- 
tions in a spirit of scientific detachment. The color line is as old as 
the United States, and we have all received impressions during our 
formative years which can hardly fail to give us an unconscious 
emotional bias. Perhaps we can gain some perspective by a realiza- 
tion that race mixture in America is inevitable and that the most 
that the anthropologist or sociologist can do is to forcast its results. 
We are by no means the first group to be confronted by these prob- 
lems, and all historical evidence goes to show that wherever two 
races remain in contact for any length of time there will be more or 
less intermixture. Indeed, it has been said that the only stock which 
would have a chance of remaining pure under such circumstances 
would be one all of whose women were too hideous to arouse the 
passions of foreign men and all of whose men were too cowardly to 
steal foreign women. Race mixture may be retarded by social pres- 
sure, but it cannot be prevented by any means of control so far 
devised. 

69 



70 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

Questions of the desirability of race mixture are inextricably 
bound up with those of racial equality. A belief in the innate super- 
iority of the group to which one belongs is probably as old as the 
human species. Most uncivilized tribes apply to themselves terms 
meaning "men" or "human beings/' with an implication that all 
other groups occupy a less exalted position. As a rule they make 
no attempt to rationalize this feeling, but we occasionally find it 
explained by myths of special favor on the part of some deity. A 
good deal of the current literature on race seems to be much the same 
sort of attempt at rationalization, although, in accordance with the 
modern European culture pattern, it is science instead of religion 
which is invoked. As the writers are Europeans, the superiority of 
the white man is accepted almost without question and the discus- 
sion revolves around the relative rank of the three European Cau- 
casic strains, Nordic, Alpine, and Mediterranean. At the present 
moment the disciples of Nordic superiority seem to have somewhat 
the best of it, but their claims have not passed unchallenged, and we 
may soon expect to see Alpine and Mediterranean champions enter- 
ing the lists against them. 

A study of the physical characteristics of the various races lends 
little support to the theory of racial inequality. If it could be shown 
that any one race was markedly nearer to the apes in its anatomy 
than the others, it would be justifiable to conclude that that race had 
lagged in evolution and was therefore lower. Racial classifications 
are based on the simultaneous occurrence of a number of traits. If 
we take these traits singly and arrange the races in a graded series 
with the apes at the bottom, we find that their positions vary with 
the criterion chosen. Thus, in the matter of hairiness, one of the most 
striking differences between man and the anthropoids, the Caucasian 
is most simian, with the Negro next and the Mongol last. Cephalic 
index places the Mongol and Alpine European below the Negro and 
Nordic, while lip-form places the Mongol lowest, with the Caucasian 
next and the Negro highest. No one race shows a significant pre- 
ponderance of simian traits, and we are forced to conclude that the 
various human breeds are simply the color phases and varieties 
which are to be expected in any mammalian species of very wide 
distribution. 



AN ANTHROPOLOGICAL VIEW OF RACE MIXTURE 71 

The whole question of the relative mental ability of the various 
races must be left an open one, for we have almost no data on which 
to base conclusions. The cultural status of the different groups can- 
not be taken as a criterion, for we know that the growth of culture 
is dependant upon many factors. Today the Japanese are competing 
with Europeans on terms of equality, yet within the memory of 
persons still living they were less advanced culturally than their 
Chinese neighbors. Moreover, history shows that sometimes one 
race and sometimes another has been in the lead. At the present 
time the Nordic European is culturally ascendent. A thousand years 
ago the Chinese were probably the most civilized people in the world. 
Two thousand years ago the palm would have been awarded to 
peoples who were predominantly of Mediterranean race. 

The only sound approach to the problem would be to make a 
large series of psychological tests on representative racial groups and 
analyze the results by statistical methods. Some work is now being 
done along this line but it will be some time before any final con- 
clusions are possible. Practically no tests have been made upon 
uncivilized groups, except a few of sense faculties, and the only ex- 
tensive data we have in regard to racial differences in intelligence 
are those derived from the tests upon the American draft during the 
late war. At their face value these tests would seem to show deep 
group distinctions. Englishmen came first, then the white draft in 
general, then Italians, then Poles, and last Negroes. An analysis 
shows, however, that the northern Negro had ten times as many 
individuals in the above-average grade and only half as many in the 
below-average grade as the southern Negro. In the literate tests he 
ranked above the Italian and only slightly below the Alabama white, 
while in the illiterate tests he surpassed the Alabama white and the 
Pole. The literate negroes of the draft, irrespective of section, 
slightly surpassed the illiterate whites. The conclusion is inevitable 
that the intelligence ratings given by these tests were much more 
dependent upon education and social opportunity than race, and 
that the differences revealed cannot be adduced as a proof of racial 
inequality. Intelligence tests are still in their infancy, but it is safe 
to say that very few of those now in use differentiate successfully 
between innate mental ability and that which is the result of train- 



72 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

ing. Moreover, they have been devised consciously or unconsciously 
from the standpoint of European culture, which at once places the 
non-European at a disadvantage. Further work may prove the ex- 
istence of racial differences in intelligence, but it seems probable 
that in these, as in physical traits, inferiority in one respect will be 
compensated for by superiority in another, so that the final result 
will be an approximate equality. 

Although there seems to be no positive evidence of physical or 
mental superiority on the part of any one race, there remains the 
possibility of qualitative differences in mentality, differences in what, 
for lack of a better term, may be called temperament. This is of 
vital importance in the problem of race mixture, for if certain races 
are temperamentally fitted for certain types of culture, it follows 
that the hybrids of such races, who will not completely inherit the 
psychological traits of either, will be less fitted to carry on the cul- 
ture of either than individuals of pure stock. The strongest evidence 
against the existence of such differences lies in the complete accul- 
turation of individuals who have been brought up as members of 
alien groups. American history is full of instances of white children, 
adopted and reared by the Indians, who became thoroughly Indian 
in culture and refused to return to their own kin. Even within the 
same race we often find cultural differences so great that the tem- 
perament theory becomes untenable. The psychological background 
of some of the American Indian cultures differed profoundly. The 
tribes of the plains and Eastern woodlands were intense individual- 
ists. Even great tribal ceremonies such as the Sun Dance or the 
initiation of the Arapaho Men's Societies always originated in in- 
dividual vows, while all war parties were undertaken on the initiative 
of some one man who usually was not a chief. There was almost no 
machinery for the coercion of members of the tribe, and their chiefs 
ruled by persuasion. The Indians of the Inca Empire, on the other 
hand, had every detail of their lives regulated by the state. The 
peasant could not leave the village of his birth unless drawn away 
for government service, and was compelled to turn in all his products 
to government magazines, from which they were redistributed ac- 
cording to the needs of the population. There was an army of officials 
each of whom had almost absolute authority over the men under 



AN ANTHROPOLOGICAL VIEW OF RACE MIXTURE 73 

him, while at the head of the great machine stood the Inca, an ab- 
solute hereditary monarch with divine attributes. The psychological 
backgrounds of these two systems have hardly a feature in common 
and it seems certain that a race which could produce both of them 
could also adapt itself to almost any cultural conditions. 

As we have no proofs of racial inequality or of racial differences 
in temperament, there is no reason to conclude a priori that racial 
hybrids will be inferior to pure individuals of either of their parent- 
stocks. A really scientific study of the results of race mixture 
is fraught with great difficulty. The ideal approach to the problem 
would be to observe hybrids of pure strains bred together under 
test conditions, but social strictures render this impossible. We are 
forced to fall back upon data obtained from chance hybrids the 
racial history of whose ancestors is usually unknown. In the case of 
American mulattoes, for example, there is hardly an instance in 
which we can tell whether the white half of an individual's ancestry 
was Nordic, Alpine, or Mediterranean, or the black half a pure 
Negro, a Hamitic Fulah, or a Negro-Malay mixture from Madagas- 
car. Under such circumstances it is not surprising that even the 
physical results of race mixture are imperfectly understood. The 
traits which go to make up a racial type appear to be inherited singly 
or in small linked groups, their propagation following Mendelian 
lines. Certain features, such as head-form and stature, seem to be 
due to a presence of several factors in the heredity, and show great 
variability, especially in the second-generation hybrids. Individuals 
or races of mixed origin will show all sorts of combinations of the 
traits of the pure ancestral strains and even with long-continued 
inbreeding will fail to evolve a uniform physical type. We have no 
evidence that hybrids or their offspring are physically inferior to 
the pure strains, and they appear to be at least equally fertile. 

At the present time it is quite impossible to establish the relative 
mental ability of hybrids and pure strains along strictly scientific 
lines. It has already been pointed out that most of our present in- 
telligence tests fail to eliminate the influences of training and cultural 
background. This places white hybrids at a marked disadvantage, 
for there are few instances in which such individuals are unreservedly 
accepted into white society or given equal opportunities with pure 



74 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

whites. The position of hybrids between diverse civilized stocks, 
such as whites and Chinese or Hindus, is especially unfortunate, for 
they are usually ostracized by both. In crosses between whites and 
uncivilized races, such as American Indians or Polynesians, the off- 
spring are often received by the less civilized group, and in such 
cases their record is usually good. Many half-breeds brought up 
among the Indians have shown unusual ability and force of charac- 
ter, and several of their best-known chiefs have been of mixed blood. 

Perhaps the best available approach to the problem of the mental 
status of hybrids lies in a study of the achievements of groups which 
we know to be racially mixed. All the great civilizations of which 
we have record appear to have been the work of thoroughly mixed 
groups. There are probably no pure races in existence at the present 
time, but relatively pure strains are nearly always backward in cul- 
ture. Taken at its face value, the evidence would seem to be all in 
favor of the hybrid. It must be remembered, however, that cultural 
contacts are the most important single factor stimulating the growth 
of civilization. Relatively pure races have remained so because they 
were isolated, and their cultural backwardness seems to be primarily 
due to this. The most that we can say is that racial hybridization 
has not interfered with the perpetuation of culture, while the cul- 
tural contacts entailed by the process have stimulated the develop- 
ment of civilizations. 

Believers in racial inequality and in the inferiority of hybrids 
may object that in most of the historic instances of race mixture 
on a large scale the stocks involved have not been very diverse. The 
three European races, for instance, all belong to the Caucasic group 
and have many traits in common which distinguish them from 
Mongols or Negroes. To find good instances of the crossing of diverse 
strains we must turn to uncivilized peoples whose racial history must 
be deduced from their present physical type. The Polynesians afford 
an excellent example. They appear to be a result of the crossing of 
at least three stocks which were branches of the Negro, Mongol, and 
Caucasic stems. These original racial types still tend to segregate 
out, but the Polynesians are unconscious of their mixed origin and 
have no social strictures based on physical traits. Physically they 
are one of the finest groups in the world, their only weakness being 
a certain lack of disease resistance, probably attributable to their long 



AN ANTHROPOLOGICAL VIEW OF RACE MIXTURE 75 

isolation. Mentally they are, I believe, as alert and intelligent as 
Europeans. Wherever they have been given a fair opportunity they 
have been quick to adopt white civilization and have competed 
successfully with the whites in the professions as well as the trades. 
There is nothing to indicate that they are inferior to any of their 
parent stocks or that hybridization has been other than beneficial 
in this instance. 

The cultural achievements of hybrid groups may not prove that 
race mixture is desirable, but they do destroy the main argument of 
those who take an alarmist attitude on the subject. There have been 
instances in which a policy of deliberate intermarriage on the part 
of the socially superior race has had excellent results. The success 
of the early Mohammedan conquests was largely due to the fact 
that they everywhere took into their harems women of the con- 
quered races. The children of such women were raised in their 
fathers' language and culture and no social line was drawn against 
them. The distinction between conquerors and conquered rapidly 
disappeared, and what the ruling group lost in purity of blood was 
more than compensated for by its gain in stability of rule. Most of 
the Arab conquests were the work of very small armies; Egypt for 
instance, was conquered by four thousand men, yet no other nation 
has been as successful in establishing its culture over a wide area and 
among racially diverse peoples. 

The so-called racial problems of the present day are actually 
social and political. That racial antipathies are altogether due to 
training is proved by the differences in the popular attitude toward 
members of the same race in different parts of the United States. 
Even the relative social rank which whites assign to the other races 
is often the result of accident. The North American feeling toward 
Indians and Negroes is a case in point. Any impartial student must 
admit that the Negro in Africa was culturally superior to the Indian 
of the United States. He had made considerable advances in all the 
arts, was an expert metal worker, and had developed forms of govern- 
ment and methods of legal procedure not unlike those of early Eu- 
rope. The African and European cultural background also had cer- 
tain features in common, for both had drawn elements from the 
ancient Mediterranean civilizations. The Indian was still in the 
Stone Age and his cultural background was utterly alien. Under 



76 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

such circumstances we should expect the white to feel a closer kin- 
ship with the Negro than with the Indian, and to accord him a great- 
er degree of social equality. As a matter of fact, the Indian is nearly 
always ranked above the Negro socially for the simple reason that 
his ancestors were free men and warriors while the Negro's ancestors, 
in America, were slaves. The closer resemblance of the Indian and 
white physical types may have somewhat influenced the Indians' 
position, but the extent to which whites and Negroes have interbred 
proves that the Negro physical type is not unattractive to the 
European. If the whites had come to know the Negroes as free men 
and the Indians as slaves the social position of the groups would no 
doubt have been reversed. 

When social distinctions on the basis of race have once become 
established they may persist for a very long time, but they are not 
proof against economic attacks. A rich member of the socially in- 
ferior group can usually find a mate among the poorer members of 
the socially superior one. The average white man has an aversion 
to contracting a regular marriage with an Indian woman, but the 
white demand for Osage women, who are rich in oil land, is now so 
great that the Indian agent requires every prospective husband to 
give a bond, submit to a physical examination, and present character 
references. White husbands have become so easy to get that they 
are now considered somewhat less desirable than even Indian men 
of other tribes. 

Culture, in the anthropological sense, is the most important fact 
of man's existence. It is the one thing which sets him apart from the 
animals. We have every reason to believe that the hybrid is as 
capable of perpetuating and improving it as the full blood and we 
may, therefore, face the fact of race mixture in America with equa- 
nimity. There is no reason to suppose that the United States of one 
hundred or five hundred years hence will be any the worse for the 
gradual absorption into its white population of the present Mongol, 
Indian, and Negro minorities. The first two are numerically unim- 
portant, while the Negroes, including those who already have a 
white admixture, form only about 10 per cent of the total. The 
process of assimilation will no doubt be a slow one, but unless all 
history is at fault it will go on until the present racial lines have been 
obliterated. Even the physical aspect of these remote future Ameri- 



DISCUSSION 77 

cans of mixed ancestry probably will not be vastly different from 
that of the present stock, for anyone who casts an anthropological 
eye over a modern American social gathering will discover some sur- 
prising things. He will see traits which bespeak a remote Mongoloid 
or Negroid strain in many persons who believe themselves to be of 
pure blood. There were Negroids in Europe as early as there were 
white men, and waves of Mongoloid peoples have swept into it since 
before the dawn of history. Even the pure "old American" is so 
hopelessly mixed racially that a little more alien blood is not likely 
to hurt him. 

DISCUSSION 



KIMBALL YOUNG 
University of Oregon 



Both Mr. Linton and Professor Reuter mention the emotional attitudes 
which members of the white race, especially in this country, take toward the 
amalgamation of races. On the other hand, we must not let our own interest in 
cultural anthropology deflect us from an honest consideration of the facts of 
race mixture. In most discussions of this problem there tends to be a denial 
not only of racial but of individual differences in mental ability within the 
racial groups. While it is true that there are no pure races today, biologically 
speaking, the facts of long inbreeding have produced certain stabilities of type 
in Mongoloid, Negroid, Malay, and white stocks. Hence we may speak of these 
as races in the general sense. It is within the period of modern history, especially 
since the seventeenth century, that great intermingling of these various stocks 
has taken place, and within the past hundred years only has this mixture gone 
on with great rapidity in certain countries where the various groups have come 
into contact with each other. 

We must admit the great difficulty in segregating the factors of innate 
ability from those of learning, for after all the matter of cultural assimilation 
comes, on the psychological level, to be a question of learning capacity. And 
learning capacity, with its central features of attention and imagination, is 
really what is meant by intellectual ability. It is a fact, too, that so far we 
possess no adequate criterion of racial differences except the statistical evidence 
based on the percentages of members of culturally comparable racial groups 
who reach or exceed the median performance of another group. Perhaps this 
statistical criterion is the only one we shall ever have. Let us examine from this 
angle some of the recent studies which have been made on individual and racial 
differences. 

Ruch has recently made an extended investigation of the learning capacity 
of three groups of persons of varying intelligence levels as measured by the best 



78 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

mental tests available. Using three types of learning material, he found that 
in perceptual-motor learning all three groups of persons, superior, average, and 
inferior, made good progress; that in perceptual-memory learning the superior 
and average groups moved, relatively, much faster than the inferior; and that 
in handling abstract, symbolic material only the superiors made any marked 
progress at all, the average group making some slight advancement and the 
inferior group practically none whatsoever, even after forty days' practice. This 
study furnishes pretty conclusive defense for individual differences. 

Peterson, working with different white, mulatto, and colored children with 
a rational learning test which lies intermediate between Ruch's second and third 
type of learning material has revealed marked differences in the performances 
of the different groups. He finds a high correlation, moreover, between the re- 
sults of his learning problem and tests of general intelligence. Both Peterson 
and Ferguson have shown a close correlation between the percentage of white 
blood in Negro-white crosses and the tests. Cultural conditions have been taken 
into account and an honest effort made to secure comparable cultural and 
educational backgrounds. Ferguson has worked with block-design tests which 
operate independently of language and formal training, and Peterson's rational 
learning and other tests are relatively free of such language factors as are frankly 
related to education. 

It may still be said, of course, that the mulatto groups have superior op- 
portunities to the pure Negro. But if, as Professor Reuter asserts, the mulattoes 
are forced to the general status of the negro rather than being allowed to partake 
of the white man's culture, then one inquires if the psychologist has not some 
point in asserting that the superiority of the mulatto over the pure-blood Negro 
is at least in part due to superior strains of white blood as well as to cultural 
factors. It must be questioned, parenthetically, whether it be true that over the 
whole period of Negro-white crossings in this country it has been the most 
mentally inferior groups of white men who have consorted with negro women. 
Certainly, up to a generation or so ago, this was not so. Furthermore, the bi- 
ologist and the psychologist may with impunity raise the query as to whether 
or note the inherent factors of superior ability may not account for the facts 
given by Professor Reuter when he says: "In every situation they [the mixed 
stocks] have erupted a percentage of intellectually capable men far in excess of 
that furnished by the native element of their racial ancestry . . . ." Especially 
does one question the completeness of the cultural theory when he remarks 
further: "In spite of the fact that the Negroes of relatively unmixed blood out- 
number the mixed bloods three or four to one, they have not produced one 
prominent man to equal ability developed by the mulattoes." This query, I 
say, is pertinent when the culturalist admits that the mulatto is, by and large, 
forced into the social-economic status of the lower-culture group. 

The same question may be raised about the Indian-white mixtures. Surely, 
whether it be true that "all sound scholarship in social phenomena assumes an 
essential equality in the native mentality of racial groups" or not, the facts of 



DISCUSSION 7.9 

marked individual differences coupled with the facts of racial amalgamation 
raise problems which the culturalist can scarcely ignore. 

Mr. Linton maintains that all higher cultures result from hybrid races, 
while the pure races do not produce the same high level of culture. We have, of 
course, a number of factors here. These include matters of the relative size of 
the groups, of large numbers versus small numbers, of the wider variability of 
ability in mixed groups and the possible biological values of outbreeding fol- 
lowed by partially selective inbreeding. There are, of course, the cultural in- 
ventions and contacts in relation to these features. Thus biological as well as 
cultural forces may well play a role in this phenomenon. 

Before concluding this review, I wish to mention some other studies which 
have been made of racial and subracial groups. In my own studies of Italian, 
Portuguese, and Spanish-Mexican children in comparison with children of the 
same age from parents of North-European ancestry, I found by correlations of 
language and non-language tests that not all the alleged differences in the abil- 
ities of these groups is due to language handicaps, as is so frequently claimed. 
The work of Miss Thomson and of other students of Terman upon Latins and 
upon Orientals bears this out. Recent studies by Yeung, Borncamp, Darsie, 
and others on Chinese and Japanese children in California, and the work of 
Smith in Hawaii indicate that the language handicap is a factor in the learning 
of the Oriental in English-speaking schools, but that this handicap only touches 
materials where language is of decided importance. In arithmetic and subjects 
where language does not operate, the Orientals equal and even excel the white 
children of older American (North-European) ancestry. On the contrary, the 
Italians and other Latins fall down in both language- and non-language- 
learning materials. 

In spite of some disagreements, as in studies by Kirkpatrick in this country 
and Gordon in London, it cannot be gainsaid that there is much evidence for 
individual differences which operate independent of cultural status, and that, 
moreover, the mixture of persons of one group with low intelligence scores with 
persons of a group who possess higher ratings makes for fixing the intelligence 
or learning capacity of the mixed-blood group at an intermediate position be- 
tween these extremes. 

To conclude, I wish to maintain that we have in the study of social phenom- 
ena to take into account three factors: (i) Those of individual differences in 
racial groups and social classes. (2) The fact of what Thomas is wont to call 
"the run of attention," or the direction of the interests and habits of the mem- 
bers of the group in question. This, in turn, depends in the first instance upon 
the learning and attending capacity of the persons concerned, and here we find 
innate differences. In addition, this run of attention depends upon what I shall 
term the third major factor in the social process : (3) This is the cultural level 
of the group, that is, the arts, crafts, sciences, folkways of the race or class. 
Until the culturalist takes the facts of individual variation into account and 
until the biologist or psychologist recognizes the tremendous place of culture in 
the development of personality and group life, we shall get nowhere. 



INTERNATIONAL ASPECTS OF THE RUSSIAN 
REVOLUTION 



JEROME DAVIS 
Yale University 



ABSTRACT 

International Aspects of the Russian Revolution. A scientific gauge for measuring 
the international aspects of the Russian Revolution would require the overcoming of 
the following obstacles: (i) its extreme complexity, (2) compartment thinking, (3) the 
bias of class and of our social heritage, (4) nationalism, (5) propaganda, (6) the warping 
incident to news transference, and (7) surface thinking with its concomitant "praise or 
blame" attitude. Eliminating secondary testimony and also primary testimony from 
those with a motive to falsify and with a habit of stereotype thinking, there is agree- 
ment on certain significant facts: (i) in the circular process of stimulation, reaction, 
and restimulation between soviet Russia and foreign countries, the policies and at- 
titudes of both have been changed, (2) the international instrumentalities established 
by the bolshevists appear much more formidable than they really are, (3) the soviet 
government has been successful in securing recognition from other governments, and 
(4) the results of this unique social experimentation have been both negative and 
positive. 

Revolutions are, next to world-warfare, the most dangerous 
social explosions known to human experience. They involve such a 
tangled skein of forces that it is extremely difficult scientifically to 
appraise them. The Russian Revolution is exceptionally compli- 
cated because the reaction itself took place in the midst of a still 
more violent international metamorphosis, the world-war, when 
group minds were so inflamed by the sentiments and passions of the 
conflict that they had neither the patience nor the mechanism to 
appraise the situation with scientific accuracy. We now know that 
in every modern war truth is the first casualty. We have not yet 
achieved a scientific gauge for measuring the international effects 
of violent social change. Yet the social scientist painstakingly exper- 
imenting, counting and measuring, has reached the place where he 
sees clearly certain of the obstacles which he must overcome if he is 
to solve this problem. As applied to the Russian problem this would 
involve among others : 

i. Its extreme complexity. Tsaristic Russia was a gigantic coun- 
try over twice as large as all Europe, with a population speaking one 

80 



TEE RUSSIAN REVOLUTION 81 

hundred different languages or dialects, changing during a revolu- 
tionary period with kaleidoscopic speed. How can the truth be 
sorted out of the jumble of conflicting testimony? 

2. Compartment thinking. We give different weights to similar 
facts. We explain away injustice in America, such as lynching or 
negro disfranchisement, because we are familiar with it; we feel that 
terror or dictatorship in Russia is in an entirely different category. 

3. The bias of class and of our social heritage. Our opinions are 
refracted by the colored glasses of our experience. To judge Russian 
institutions by American concepts is completely to misconceive the 
situation. 

4. Nationalism.- We are unwilling to face the truth that our 
nation may have been just as stupid in its treatment of the Revolu- 
tion as the Revolution has been in treating us. 

5. Propaganda. The Bolsheviks have received more free ad- 
vertising in a shorter space of time than any other political party 
since world-history began, but it has largely been false. Dr. E. A. 
Ross dedicates a book, "To my fellow-Americans who have become 
weary of being fed lies and propaganda about Russia." Once an anti- 
Bolshevik complex was firmly established in the public mind, Ameri- 
cans have enjoyed having their prejudices played upon. Hence it be- 
came profitable to retail every sort of slander about the Bolshevik 
government from "their deadly and dangerous red propaganda" to 
the "nationalization of women" story. 

6. The warping incident to news transference. While we have 
annihilated distance today by making communication instantane- 
ous, in so doing we have enormously increased the chances of group 
misunderstandings. If the Bolsheviks should be overthrown to- 
morrow, the world would hear of it instantly and be powerfully af- 
fected by it. On the other hand, few people would know much of 
what actually happened. A piece of news has to run the gamut of a 
good many hands, each of which may unconsciously remake the item. 
As far as the scientific appraisal of the facts of the occurrence is con- 
cerned, we are still cut off by an Atlantic Ocean of prejudices and 
misinformation. This is just as true of the news Soviet Russia re- 
ceives about America as of what America receives about Soviet 
Russia. 



82 TDE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

7. Surface thinking with its concomitant emotional "praise or 
blame" attitude. Because of mental superficiality we tend to moral- 
ize about particular revolutionary actors instead of understanding 
the situations which produced them. 

It is but a short step from emotional astigmatism to rationali- 
zation and unconscious deception. In the material world if something 
is radically wrong with a manufactured product, we do not destroy 
the article or burn the factory. Similarly, with serious social defects 
we should scientifically investigate rather than use blockades or 
armies. As a matter of fact, the cooler scientific judgment of the 
world now recognizes that the cause for the violent social explosion 
in Russia was the terrible maladjustment within the Tsar's empire. 
Conditions had been sociologically unsanitary and dangerous for a 
long age-span. Ever since the warning revolutionary stimuli of 1905, 
the world was apprised of the situation. 

In spite of the difficulties it is possible to reach certain conclu- 
sions regarding the international effects of the Soviet experiment. 
In making such a searching scientific analysis it is necessary first to 
sort out agreements between all those witnesses who are competent. 
As Dr. Giddings has so admirably reminded us in his Scientific Study 
of Society, this involves eliminating the following: 

1. Hearsay or secondary testimony. As regards Russia this 
throws out all those Russians, such as Ambassador Bakmetieff, who 
have not been in Russia since the Bolsheviks assumed the power 
over seven years ago. 

2. Those who have a motive to falsify. This eliminates all those 
who lost position or property in Russia or who have been expelled 
from the country, as well as the individuals belonging to the Com- 
munist party. 

3. Those who are controlled by a complex, stereotype, or mastering 
idea. This eliminates certain "parlor" socialists both pro- and anti- 
Bolshevik. 

Eliminating all of the above testimony and including only those 
who are intellectually competent, we find agreement on certain very 
significant facts: 

First, there was a circular process of stimulation, reaction, and re- 
stimulation between Soviet Russia and foreign countries, and both 
were being changed by the process, Soviet Russia probably more 



THE RUSSIAN REVOLUTION 83 

than the rest of the world. One international effect of the policies of 
the Soviet government was intervention by the Allies. There was 
the attack from the north with American and Allied soldiers; from 
the east with American, Japanese, and Czecho-Slovak soldiers; 
from the south by Denikin with Allied aid; from the south by Wran- 
gel with Allied aid; from the west by Udenitch with Allied aid; and 
again from Poland with French bayonets. All of these processes 
changed the policies and action of Russia, and again these same re- 
actions changed the policies and attitudes of the foreign countries. 
The Bolshevik government at first tried to keep peace with the Allies 
and secure recognition from them, as the testimony of the American 
ambassador, Francis, and Colonel Robins shows. Failing in this, 
after intervention had set in, the Bolshevik government enacted new 
and more radical decrees which markedly changed its behavior. 
Similarly in America, public sentiment against intervention grew 
to overwhelming proportions in those states which had a large num- 
ber of American soldiers fighting in Russia against the Bolsheviks. 
Here we have clearly the familiar phenomena of interstimulation and 
response, of imitation, of suggestion, and of new group-reactions to 
a new stimulus. 

Second, the Bolsheviks have established international instru- 
mentalities: 

a) The Union of Socialist Soviet Republics. Composed of White 
Russia, Ukraine, Transcaucasia, Azerbaijan, Armenia, Georgia, and 
Soviet Russia, which itself includes ten autonomous republics and 
eleven autonomous areas. 

V) The Communist International.- This organization would wel- 
come within each state "an armed struggle for the overthrow of the 
international Bourgeoisie and the establishment of an international 
Soviet republic as a transition to the complete abolition of the capi- 
talist state. " As time has gone on, the Russian governmental au- 
thorities have been less and less inclined to interfere in the internal 
affairs of other countries and more inclined to center all their efforts 
on holding and strengthening their position in Russia. At the Fourth 
Congress held in Moscow in December, 1922, there were delegates 
from all the leading countries of the world. The total membership 
was supposed to be over one million. 

c) The Red International of Labor Unions. An organization of 



84 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

all the trade unions throughout the world who believe sufficiently 
in Communism to join. They claim a membership of over twelve 
millions. 

d) The Communist Youth International.- This apparatus is de- 
signed to bind together the youth of the world who believe in Com- 
munism. It apparently has not had much success outside of Russia, 
for the total membership does not exceed 800,000, composed almost 
exclusively of Russians. 

These agencies used by politicians to terrorize the gullible, ap- 
pear very much more formidable than they actually are. For exam- 
ple, one outstanding leader of the Japanese trade unions privately 
stated that although they send representatives to the Red Inter- 
national they do not believe in Communism, and they also send dele- 
gates to the non-Communist international trade-union conferences. 
Their total membership is included as within the Moscow fold, but 
actually very few of their members believe in Communism. The 
fact is that as far as the world-branches of Communism are con- 
cerned they are largely a paper organization. They have members in 
every country, it is true, but little real strength. Their actual threat 
to the rest of the world lies chiefly in the extent to which, either with- 
in a nation or in its treatment by powerful outsiders, there exists 
such gross injustice and stupidity as to drive members into the 
Communist ranks on any terms. The extent to which Communism 
thrives in India, Germany, or elsewhere is to some degree a barome- 
ter of maladjustment, and hence of utility value. 

Third, the Soviet government has been successful in securing 
recognition from other governments. Practically every important 
nation in the world has accorded such recognition except the United 
States, which cannot permanently maintain its exclusiveness. It 
seems probable that recognition has been accorded because in the 
complicated interdependence of our "great society" the isolation of 
such a large division as that of Russia is extremely difficult and even 
damaging to our interrelated and delicate financial mechanisms. 

Fourth, as is true in the case of nearly all unique and startling 
social experimentation, there has been both a negative and a posi- 
tive result. Any appraisal must still be largely subjective. It is im- 
possible in a brief compass to list all the effects. On the debit side of 



THE RUSSIAN REVOLUTION 85 

the ledger, among others, we have the age-long familiar practices of 
dictatorship by the few, suppression of freedom of speech and of 
press, still utilized in every country at times. The execution of polit- 
ical prisoners has ceased. There is the hostility of the government 
to religion, which no doubt has both a good and a bad effect. Al- 
though primarily an internal problem, this has its international 
ramifications and consequences. The Roman Catholic church has 
only recently taken cognizance of this policy by a strong declara- 
tion. There is no important governmental party outside of the Bol- 
shevik which proclaims itself as frankly atheistic, nor is there any 
other group which attempts so vigorously to oppose certain of the 
religious values and superstitions and to substitute for these sci- 
ence, morality, and its own brand of culture. Within the nation 
there has also been the application of untried theories on a large 
scale by untrained and consequently incompetent administrators, 
with a consequent social wastage. 

It is frequently charged that the Bolsheviks advocate peace and 
then maintain a colossal army. This accusation is unfounded. In 
proportion to population her army has only one soldier for every 200 
inhabitants, whereas Poland has one for every 100, Roumania, one 
for every 82, France, one for every 60, and Finland including her 
civic guards one to every 28. When we recall that Russia must be 
prepared to face attack from all these countries, not to mention 
Japan, and that just two nations alone Poland and France with a 
population half as great as Russia, together maintain a larger stand- 
ing army, it can readily be seen how difficult it is for the Soviet to 
reduce her army further unless other nations do likewise. 

On the positive side, we have the fact that the Bolsheviks have 
so far completely prevented the return of the Tsar's autocracy, one 
of the most dangerous foes to democracy which the world faced in 
1914. The land has also irrevocably been distributed to the peasants. 
The Bolsheviks have largely prevented the dismemberment of Rus- 
sia and have recreated a great federated nation. Such a patriotic 
Russian as General Brusiloff, formerly commander-in-chief of the 
Russian armies, in talking to me in Moscow in 1921, reiterated time 
and again that while he was opposed to the Bolsheviks, he knew that 
they had prevented foreign countries from seizing her resources, and 



86 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

that as between Russian Bolshevism and foreign exploitation he 
would choose the Soviet government every time. Russian propaganda 
undoubtedly helped to bring on the German revolution. I myself 
made a study of some of their propaganda work at first hand when, 
at the request of the American government, I sent into the German 
lines from Russia a million copies of President Wilson's Fourteen 
Points of Peace, a work which necessitated receiving official permis- 
sion from the Bolshevik war minister. General Ludendorff of the 
German staff says that Bolshevik propaganda was the chief factor 
in bringing on the collapse of Germany. General Hoffman says, "It 
was Bolshevik propaganda that routed Germany from the East and 
then broke her morale and gave us this defeat and this revolution 
you now see ruining us." In the publication of secret treaties the 
Soviet government did the world an inestimable service, and may 
have helped to sound the death knell of that ancient device. While 
it is popular to point to the damaging effects of the Bolshevik advo- 
cacy of violence, we must not overlook the stimulus which the So- 
viet government gave, perhaps unconsciously, to the positive forces 
in the labor world. A number of the labor leaders in England have 
said that the consciousness that in Russia the workers had seized 
the power enormously helped the English labor party in appealing 
to the workers for support. It has certainly caused labor leaders in 
both England and America to examine carefully their program and 
tactics, to see that they were not committing the mistakes of Bol- 
shevism. Recall the statements of Ramsay MacDonald and Samuel 
Gompers. We can safely conclude that there has been both a good 
and a bad influence of Bolshevism, and that we are not yet in a posi- 
tion to draw up the balance sheet. 

Finally, we cannot appraise the Russian Revolution without 
comparing the motives of her leaders with those of our own. We 
have seen that while the Communist International does want to de- 
stroy capitalism, capitalism in its turn desires to destroy the Com- 
munist International. We should recognize that there are real dif- 
ferences in economic theory between the Russian rulers and the rest 
of the world. The Soviet government desires to build a nation in 
which all factories, mines, railways, and other means of production 
and distribution are owned by the people. Nevertheless a great deal 



TBE RUSSIAN REVOLUTION 87 

of their differences are a mere matter of words and revolutionary 
formulae. The international practices of Bolshevism are not as bad 
as some of their phrases and may be worse than others. We should 
get down below mere verbiage to the genuine likenesses and differ- 
ences. When we do this we find that Russia no less than other na- 
tions desires the economic and intellectual well-being of her people. 
Americans should be no more alarmed over red language in the 
Communist International than of the bombastic language at a na- 
tional political convention in our own land. A considerable amount 
of politics is to be found in both. We should distinguish also between 
the violently radical Bolsheviks and the rest of the party. The num- 
ber enrolled in the various third internationals are like those in a 
third political party; many of them are merely liberals. The large 
majority do not even understand the real economic theory of Com- 
munism. In the early days of the Russian Revolution, the Bolshe- 
viks may have been primarily interested in a world revolution; they 
now know that if Bolshevism is to endure, it must first make a suc- 
cessful demonstration in its own territory. In order to be successful, 
the problem of economic production must be solved. As Americans 
we are interested in the same problem; we should be able to co- 
operate at this point. The fact is that millions of dollars' worth of 
international trade is going back and forth between Russia and the 
rest of the world now. 

In conclusion, while recognizing that we are not yet ready to 
pronounce historic judgments, we should endeavor as sociologists to 
look at the Russian Revolution from the long-range point of view. 
We are tempted to consider it in the light of the period since the 
Revolution. This inevitably warps our perspective. In reality we 
should remember that Bolshevism, including its international activi- 
ties, is the logical reaction of the stimuli arising from the centuries 
of oppression under the Tsar's autocracy. It is too early to appraise 
the international effects of the explosive compound which has re- 
sulted, but it is safe to say that it has both constructive and destruc- 
tive features. In the light of history it may well be that the phe- 
nomena of the Russian Revolution will be considered among the 
major developments of the twentieth century. 



SOCIOLOGICAL FACTORS IN THE INTERPRETATION OF 
INTERNATIONAL RELATIONS: WITH SPECIFIC IL- 
LUSTRATIONS FROM SOUTHEASTERN EUROPE AND 
THE NEAR EAST 



EARLE EDWARD EUBANK 

University of Cincinnati 



ABSTRACT 

Sociological Factors in the Interpretation of International Relations. Science, so far 
as it has gone, indicates that the forces of the cosmos fall into two classes: those that 
make toward unification, and those that make against it; and that on the whole the 
trend has been toward the triumph of the former. This has been manifested in human 
society as well as in the physical world. Mankind has progressed from the primitive 
family as the unit of sovereignty to the modern nation, at each transition stage success- 
fully solving the problems of adjustment peculiar to it. We are now in the stage of 
transition from nationalism to internationalism. This paper undertakes to indicate the 
problems peculiar to this transition stage by analysis of the factors geographic, bio- 
logic, and social responsible for international disunity as illustrated in Southeastern 
Europe. The conclusion is reached that the likenesses of men throughout the world are 
primary, and constitute a rational and fundamental basis for ultimate human unity; 
that their unlikenesses are secondary, and must not be regarded as constituting in- 
evitable and permanent grounds for division. 



A law of polarity seems to pervade the known world. The nega- 
tive electrode stands over against the positive. The radiant ten- 
dencies of matter run counter to those which are gravitational. For 
every action there is an equal and an opposite reaction. The ten- 
dency to withdraw is matched by the tendency to approach. Kata- 
bolism vs. anabolism; dominance vs. recessiveness; repulsion vs. 
attraction; disintegration vs. integration: in every realm of science 
we recognize the conflict between the forces which make for unity 
and those which make for disunity. From one point of view the 
whole great general process of nature has been one of gradual sub- 
ordination of the disunifying forces to those which unify. 

Of the many trends discernible in the development of that cur- 
ious thing we call civilization, one of the most striking is that of the 
increasing integration of its parts: group life tends toward enlarge- 
ment. From the family to the clan, to the tribe, to the nation, to 



INTERPRETATION OF INTERNATIONAL RELATIONS 89 

federations of nations, the process of enlargement has gone on, 
marked at every step by the integration of a larger and larger num- 
ber of constituent segments, and by their closer correlation. 

This tendency is not peculiar to the social world. Long ago nat- 
ural science pointed out that a principle of aggregation upon some 
basis of selection inherent in the aggregating particles is operative 
in the cosmosphere and the biosphere. Aggregations of correlated 
atoms compose molecules. Aggregations of correlated molecules 
compose masses. Aggregations of protoplasmic units under certain 
conditions compose life-cells. Aggregations of cells under certain 
conditions compose morphological bodies which, in their highest 
form, are human beings. The same general principle carried into 
the sociosphere accounts for that group life which it is the especial 
province of sociology to study. 

Every stage in the long journey in social development from 
Pithecanthropus Erectus until our own day has been marked by 
certain constantly recurring features. Among these have been (i) 
the breaking down of former isolations; (2) the discovery of mutual 
likenesses, and an emphasis upon them rather than upon differences; 

(3) the discovery of the practical advantages of co-operative effort; 

(4) the discovery of consensual methods of adjustment, and their 
substitution for conflict methods; (5) the development of group 
structural forms competent to accommodate the enlarged body; 
(6) the development of a technique of action suitable to the inter- 
play of the larger number of functional parts. The whole of these 
together may be characterized as an increasing movement toward unity 
of larger and larger masses. Wherever along the line a stable group 
life comes to prevail it means that some affective basis of unification 
has been found. Where it does not prevail the elements of division 
are still paramount. The smallest possible social grouping, that of 
two persons, must be accompanied by these features, as must the 
largest; and all the stages in between, if it is to endure. 

At every juncture in group life of any kind the ultimate social 
problem is that of living together. This has been clearly pointed 
out by Professor Ellwood in his volume The Social Problem. Group 
life is at its best where this problem is most fully met. Other things 
being equal, the adequacy of group life for the needs of its members 



9 o THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

varies in the degree with which this problem has been mastered. And 
obviously where the social integers (be they nations or individual 
persons) cannot live together, they cannot have group life. In other 
words, a social grouping of any sort is possible only when its con- 
stituent members have mastered this problem to some practical ex- 
tent. The growth of human organization itself one of the most 
conspicuous trends of civilization is a constant progression in the 
discovery of larger effective bases of unity. 

All of this, simple as it is when stated, is vital to an understand- 
ing of international relations. Herein reside the sociological factors 
whose comprehension is essential to an analysis of situations between 
nations as between persons. For the first thing to be pointed out in 
approaching such a question is this: that sociologically international 
problems are nothing new nor different from those already familiar 
in the smaller and more easily studied human groups. Their differ- 
ences from those of smaller groups are not of kind, but wholly of 
degree. They are more involved and intricate, because the machin- 
ery is more complex. They are sometimes more subtle, being hidden 
in a larger mass. They are more difficult of control by reason of 
their dimensions and momentum. But intrinsically they are the 
same. 

The object of this paper is to present certain of these sociological 
factors as they are found in relations between nations. The field of 
illustration which has been selected is that of Southeastern Europe 
and such of the Near East as focuses about the Balkan area. Per- 
haps no other part of the world could better serve as an exhibit for 
the purpose. Not merely of recent years, but from time immemorial 
this arena has been one of unimaginable hatreds and strife. Herein 
have been concentrated factors of confusion little comprehended by 
$ie Western Hemisphere, whose variety and intensity make this an 
exhibit of unusual sociological value. 

In this disturbed part of the world the divisive elements have so 
far had the ascendancy over those making for cohesion. Both intra- 
nationally and internationally this is strikingly true. We may ex- 
amine the bases of the disunity of the Balkan peoples under three 
familiar headings: the geographic, the biologic (including ethnic), 
and the social. 



INTERPRETATION OF INTERNATIONAL RELATIONS 91 
I. GEOGRAPHIC FACTORS OF BALKAN DISUNITY 

Both by geographic location and by topography the Balkan 
peninsula seems to have been predestined to conflict. Separated 
from Asia only by the silver thread of the Bosporus and the Dar- 
danelles, it has formed a natural bridge for the surging waves of 
migratory movement between the two continents. Although it is 
integrally a part of Europe, its great rivers and their wide-mouthed 
valleys have stood as open and inviting doorways to the restless 
Asiatic populations. At the same time her mountain walls have 
separated her from her own European hinterland. What more nat- 
ural, therefore, than that the fierce hordes of Asia, restive from 
famines and pressure of rival peoples, from long before the days of 
Attila should have accepted her geographic invitation to enter? And 
having entered, that they should be met by resistance from the 
equally fierce mid-European peoples who resented this invasion? 
Furthermore, the internal structure of the region, intersected in 
every direction as it is by mountain ranges, has formed natural 
pockets and corners, which, once occupied, isolated its inhabitants 
from each other, and so led to the accentuation of their differences. 
Therefore the tendency has been in the direction of separation of 
life, with each group living in a world of its own, hostile to all other 
groups, and averse to any unification. 

II. BIOLOGIC FACTORS OF BALKAN DISUNITY 

The ethnic diversity in this part of the world is a Chinese puzzle. 
European has crossed with Asiatic at a dozen ethnic juncture points; 
Nordic has crossed with Mediterranean, and Slav has interpenetrated 
both in innumerable combinations, till any racial designation what- 
ever has ceased to have meaning. The Bulgar is Alpine Slav mingled 
with Asiatic, Finnish, or Turkish blood. The Greek is Hellenic mixed 
with Nordic, changing to Mediterranean strains. The Roumanian, 
who claims to be Latin (something racially non-existent), is in reality 
Slav grafted onto Mediterranean and overlaid with Asiatic. The 
Hungarian is Magyar, which is Alpine plus Nordic plus Slav plus a 
dominant Asiatic element which almost conceals the first two. The 
Turk, who colors so many of the groups, is Mongolo-Tartar. While 
the Slavic element itself a mixture is the one most prominent 



92 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

throughout the region. A census would reveal a greater conglomerate 
than that noted on the day of Pentecost: Armenian, Bulgar, Croat, 
Dalmation, German, Greek, Gypsy, Jew, Magyar, Montenegran, 
Roumanian, Russian, Slav, Slovene, Turk, each in itself an inextric- 
able composite of still other composites. 

The evidence shows overwhelmingly that there exists little bio- 
logical basis for national distinctions, yet there exists in every group 
mind an almost ineradicable notion that it is a sharply defined race, 
and, of course, superior to all other racial groups in its very physical 
essence. 

III. SOCIAL FACTORS OF BALKAN DISUNITY 

Many different parts of the world have contributed to the kalei- 
doscopic social background of the Balkans. Each people that has 
entered the territory has brought in its own language and culture 
and traditions as well as its racial self. These have been intensified 
by the isolation enforced by topographic barricades. Where there 
has been enough contact to allow interplay, these have produced 
other combinations, which have still further contributed to diversi- 
fication of social heritage. Linguistically, they are represented by a 
score of tongues. Religiously, they pray in the name of Moses, of 
Mohammed, and of Jesus. Of the Christian group, those whose an- 
cestors came most under the influence of Rome give allegiance to the 
Vatican, those whose forefathers were under Byzantine influence 
belong to the Greek Orthodox church, while those reaching nearest 
to Central Europe reflect the Reformation in various Protestant 
sects. Culturally, Rome and Byzantium still divide this part of 
the world between them, except where the scimitar of Mohammed 
has expelled both. 

Sharper than the differences of language, religion, and culture 
are those produced by a historic past filled with mutual injuries, 
antagonisms, and strifes which give to the present generation a 
legacy of hatreds long since become traditional. In the many and 
dramatic turns of fortune's wheel a part, at least, of each of these 
states has at some time or other been under the domination of some 
other of these states. In a series of wars and their subsequent treaties 
which even a trained historian finds it hard to follow, shifts in boun- 



INTERPRETATION OF INTERNATIONAL RELATIONS 93 

dary lines have followed one another so rapidly as to be a map- 
maker's despair. 

Every shift of borderline is a new Alsace-Lorraine to breed bitter 
dreams and plans of vengeance on the part of the loser, no matter 
how just may be the basis of transfer. Bulgaria mourns Dobrudja 
to the northeast and Macedonia to the south. Greece, although en- 
larged at the expense of Turkey and Bulgaria, mourns Epirus. The 
diminished remnant of Hungary can never forget that two-thirds of 
her territory and people were torn from her side. And Turkey, once 
the overlord of most of Southeastern Europe, considers its redistri- 
bution as robbery. 

Furthermore, each territorial alteration has placed under the 
authority of the winning nation a minority population taken from 
some other. These minorities, despite the assurances of the treaties, 
have suffered the usual fate of minorities at the hands of their con- 
querors. Overt abuse, above and beyond the commonplace denial 
of ordinary justice, has been the lot of these border-zone residents. 
Thousands have had no recourse but to abandon homes and prop- 
erty indemnified only to a fractional part of the true value and 
seek refuge in what Venizelos of Greece calls "voluntary migration," 
beyond the shifted frontiers. Homeless by the change, impoverished 
to the point of starvation, angered to desperation by what they have 
undergone, these "voluntary" migrants constitute a perpetual seed- 
bed of international disturbance along the boundaries, their feelings 
shared by their nation as a whole. (In Bulgaria alone nearly loper 
cent of the population is composed of refugees from beyond her 
present borders.) Those who have not migrated and it is impossible 
for all to do so remain as unassimilated portions of a country to- 
ward which they are alien and hostile. (Roumania, for example, now 
has a total population of about seventeen millions, six millions of 
whom are non-Roumanians, chiefly from enemy countries. Another 
five million are "redeemed" Roumanians, who were subjects of 
other countries before the treaties added them to the revamped 
territory, but whose background of life and experience there was so 
different as to make them practically foreigners upon restoration to 
their own land.) 



94 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

Perhaps this illustration (which might be interminably con- 
tinued) has been carried far enough to drive home the point it was 
intended to convey. 

The Balkans constitute an example par excellence of internation- 
al disunity. Geographically, ethnically, linguistically, religiously, cul- 
turally, traditionally, politically, they stand apart from one another. 
Not only that, but in the realignment of boundaries, sections which 
would seem to belong together have been sundered; others have been 
unwisely wedded and seem already promising candidates for divorce. 
The treaty-makers, who declared themselves determined to remake 
Europe along lines of "natural" cohesion, were not equal to the task. 
Mr. Lloyd George himself is quoted as confessing the failure in saying 
recently, "We have Balkanized all that part of Europe." 

Now if sociology has any message for the field of international 
relations which should be emphasized more than another, it is one 
that may be expressed in terms of the familiar geometric axiom: 
The whole is greater than any of its parts. And correspondingly, the 
interests of the whole are superior to the particular interests of any 
of its subdivisions. Hence, adjustment among human beings can 
never be considered final until it includes all men who have knowl- 
edge of one another. Nationalism, therefore, cannot be considered 
the final word in the Balkans or anywhere else in the world, because 
it thinks in terms of parts rather than of the whole. Any thinking 
we do in this field is incomplete which fails to include all mankind. 

This carefully reasoned conclusion of sociology is tantamount to 
saying that there exist among all men fundamental bases of unity. This 
sociology firmly believes. It points to the logic of history, which 
reveals the unmistakable trend in this direction. It is a far cry from 
Cro-Magnon to a modern nation; but every step of the enlargement 
of the unit of political sovereignity has been the result of the dis- 
covery of mutual interests and likenesses that transcend the more 
immediately obvious differences and unlikenesses. Accepting the 
witness of the age-long human record, sociology says to all nations: 
The likenesses of men throughout the world are primary and constitute 
a fundamental basis for unity; their unlikenesses are secondary, and 
must not be regarded as constituting inevitable and permanent grounds 



INTERPRETATION OF INTERNATIONAL RELATIONS 95 

for division. This applies to international, as well as to lesser, re- 
lationships. 

Let us clearly understand wherein human unity really resides. 
It is not found in geographic, nor biological, nor social conditions 
per se. It does not intrinsically reside in ties of kinship, nor in iden- 
tity of language, nor religion, nor culture, nor territorial proximity. 
These things are not unity. They are merely conditions which, under 
certain circumstances, may give rise to unity. Unity itself is a psych- 
ic thing, a mental kinship. In the last analysis it is a harmonious 
mutual social attitude. Sociology must therefore tell the makers of 
treaties that they cannot create enduring states by a mere shifting 
of boundary lines. Even the deeply imbedded idea that ethnic iden- 
tity constitutes in itself a "natural" basis of nationality must be 
discarded in the light of such facts as Southeastern Europe exhibits. 
The effective power to bind together the members of any group must 
come from within not from without. 

The substance of the foregoing pages may be epitomized in a 
series of statements: 

1. The known forces of the universe fall into two divisions: (a) Those mak- 
ing toward harmonious unification of parts, and (b) those making against it. 

2. On the whole, a distinctly discernible trend is noted toward the predomi- 
nance of the former. 

3. This same fact in human society manifests itself in the form of the con- 
stant enlargement of sovereign groups. Larger and larger masses of people are 
finding effectual bases for co-operative relationships. 

4. The politico-social ideal should be that of all mankind harmoniously 
united and working consciously toward common ends. The necessary subdivi- 
sions of human kind should be merely those of differentiated parts properly 
geared into an effectively working whole, and not those of conflicting minor parts. 

5. The larger the groups to be united, the more difficult is the solution of 
the problem, because the elements involved are more numerous, diverse, and com- 
plicated (Southeastern Europe has been offered by way of graphic illustration 
and partial explanation of this fact). But except in matter of complexity the 
stage of international unity, still ahead of us, does not differ from the simpler 
stages which we have already attained. Therefore, this problem however dif- 
ficult is not insoluble. 

6. But human unity, whatever the group, is essentially a psychic thing. It 
must rest upon the mutually recognized mental kinship of its members. 



96 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

This mental kinship cannot be wrought mechanically by accident 
of territorial proximity. Ethnic and cultural similarities, important 
as they are, do not automatically constitute it. Mere political ag- 
glomeration will not, in and of itself, achieve it. The methods to be 
employed in its creation are not new. They are the ones made long 
since familiar in developing a national consciousness in our own 
heterogeneous American population: viz., (i) universal education, 
in order that the population may have a cultural basis of likeness; 
and (2) universal intercommunication, in order that people may 
become aware of that likeness. 

Any method which leaves these out of account is foredoomed to 
failure. 



POPULATION DENSITIES AND THE IMMIGRATION 
POLICY OF THE UNITED STATES 



NILES CARPENTER 
University of Buffalo 



ABSTRACT 

Population Density and the Immigration Policy of the United States^. A comparison 
of the density of population in various countries shows that the United States has a 
much smaller population than other regions, as Southeastern Asia and Western Europe, 
which are in the same zone of optimum climatic conditions, and relatively to climate and 
resources, is less densely populated than those other regions in Europe and Asia outside 
the zone of optimum climatic conditions. So long as this condition continues, there will 
exist, as between the United States and the rest of the world, a state of unstable popula- 
tion equilibrium arising from the human tendency to move from more densely crowded 
to those less densely crowded regions which are as well situated as the more densely 
populated regions or better situated than them a condition which the United States 
fulfils. Moreover, the multiplication of economic opportunities offered by a relatively 
sparsely populated area in the present industrialist period adds a powerful incentive 
for migration toward the United States. 

Nevertheless, this unstable population equilibrium is being deliberately maintained 
by the present immigration policy of the United States, which by means of quota limita- 
tions and exclusion acts, seeks to maintain its favorable position by restricting or barring 
migrants from countries that are, absolutely or relatively, more densely populated. 

Interference with the social forces controlling migration is bound to react profoundly 
upon the populations affected. In the present case, such a reaction would almost cer- 
tainly give rise to widespread resentment, possibly to diplomatic and economic reprisals, 
and conceivably to conflict, provided the countries concerned could achieve unity of 
action. In such a conflict the relatively sparse population density of the United States 
would not prove an advantage, for it would be overwhelmingly outnumbered by those 
countries whose populations were affected by its immigration policy. 

It is possible, but not probable, that a world-wide slackening in the birth-rate 
might indefinitely postpone the friction arising out of the American immigration policy. 
It is also possible that a "safety valve" might be provided by emigration to the Southern 
Hemisphere, or even to the polar and subpolar regions, but such an event would have 
to wait upon scientific discoveries, particularly relating to health and energy in tropical 
and polar regions, which are not at present in sight. An increased birth-rate in the 
United States would restore equilibrium, but friction would arise before this became 
effective. The situation would, of course, also be altered by a modification of the immi- 
gration policy of this country. 



On July i, 1924, the United States, after four years of experi- 
mentation and controversy, definitely embarked upon a restrictive 
immigration policy as regards Europe, the Near East, and Africa, 
and extended its policy of exclusion as regards the Orient. A sum- 
mary review of the trend of immigration before and after the adop- 

97 



98 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

tion of this new policy serves to demonstrate how drastic it is and 
how profoundly it already has begun to react upon the migratory 
tendencies of the world's population. During the decade igoo-igio, 1 
the net immigration to this country the bulk of it from Europe 
was about 5,365,000, or slightly under 55 per cent of the natural 
increase of the native population. During the three months follow- 
ing the beginning of the operation of the present immigration law, 
the net immigration was at the rate of 3,244,320 per decade, of which 
over half, or 1,898,310, represented immigration from Mexico, Can- 
ada, and other portions of the Western Hemisphere which are not 
now quota-limited, but which as the Secretary of Labor has re- 
cently pointed out 2 will presently have to be put under a quota to 
make the new restrictive policy completely effective. The total, ex- 
clusive of the immigration from the Western Hemisphere, was at 
the rate of only 1,346,010 per decade, or but about 13 per cent of 
the natural increase of the native population during the decade 
1910-20. 

There seem to have been two general motives underlying the 
very widespread popular sentiment to which this new policy gave 
expression. On the one hand, there was a belief that various nation- 
ality and racial groups were unsuitable for amalgamation with the 
American stock, and should, therefore, be restricted, or as in the 
case of the Japanese altogether excluded. On the other hand, there 
was the opinion that it was necessary drastically to reduce the in- 
crease in the American population attributable to immigration, in 
order to forestall the onset of an overcrowded population and the 
social and economic evils resulting therefrom. It is with this second 
feature of the American immigration policy that this paper is con- 
cerned. 

The discussion falls under three heads: first, the population den- 
sity of the United States as compared both absolutely and relatively 
with that of Europe and Asia; second, the effect of the comparative 

1 The decade 1910-20 was so affected by the world-war that it is valueless for com- 
parative purposes. The figures for net imigration 19001910, and for population increase 
1900-1920 are from Rossiter's Census monograph, Increase of Population. The data on 
net immigration since July, 1924, are calculated from the current bulletins of the 
United States Commissioner of Immigration. 

2 Associated Press dispatch, December 8, 1924, summarizing Annual Report. 



POPULATION AND THE IMMIGRATION POLICY OF U.S. 99 

population densities of the United States and these other regions 
upon the population equilibrium of the world; and third, the re- 
action upon the populations of these regions of the new American 
immigration policy. 

Before proceeding to the development of the first topic, it is 
necessary to distinguish between absolute and relative population 
density. Absolute population density refers simply to the number of 
people dwelling in a given unit of land area thus, so many per 
square mile or per square kilometer. Relative population density 
refers to the number of people dwelling in a given unit of land area 
compared with the number of people whom that area is capable of 
supporting. Thus, Arabia, with an absolute density of 4.1 per square 
kilometer, probably has a greater relative density than Iowa, whose 
absolute density is 16.5 per square kilometer. It need scarcely be 
added that absolute population density can be computed exactly, 
whereas relative density can be estimated only within broad limits. 

Examination of the data relating to population density makes it 
clear that the United States has an absolute population density very 
much below that of similarly circumstanced regions in Europe and 
Asia, and that it probably also has a relative density lower than that 
existing in many of the less favorably circumstanced areas of these 
continents. Before proceeding to an examination of the statistical 
materials, however, it is necessary to indicate what countries are 
similarly circumstanced to the United States and what countries 
are less favorably circumstanced. To do so requires an excursus into 
the domain of climatology. 1 

The United States is situated in what might be termed the belt 
of optimum climatic endowment. This belt includes all of the United 
States excepting the gulf coast and the arid West and Southwest; 
Southern Canada; West-Central Europe, excepting Northern Scan- 
dinavia; Southeastern Asia, particularly Japan; and, in the Southern 

1 The writer is relying here chiefly upon Huntington and Cushing's Principles of 
Human Geography, and Huntington's Civilization and Climate. The maps are taken 
from these works, and also from Bartholomew's Atlas of Economic Geography, and from 
Howarth's A Commercial Geography of the World, excepting the maps on population 
density which is from the Annuaire Statisque. The tables on population density and 
total population are from the Aperqu Annuel de la Demographic for 1922, while those 
on production are from the Commerce Yearbook for 1923. The writer is indebted to 
Professor F. W. Willcox of Cornell University for the loan of the Annuaire Statistique 
and the Aper$u Annuel de la Demographic. 



ioo THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

Hemisphere, New Zealand, parts of Argentina and Chile, and the 
southeastern tip of Australia. 

Three major factors enter into the superior climatic endowment 
of the United States and these other regions: first, temperature; 
second, precipitation and humidity; and third, variety. They affect 
human life, and, pari passu, population density in two ways: first, 
in the type and amount of agricultural products yielded; second, in 
the health and energy attainable by man for the prosecution of his 
daily tasks and for the adaptation of the environment to his needs. 

As regards precipitation and humidity, the belt under discussion 
is provided with a moderate amount of rainfall spread fairly evenly 
throughout the year. Moreover, as contrasted with the Asiatic mon- 
soon area, this region is assured of a fairly certain supply of moisture 
from year to year. As regards temperature, this belt, while experi- 
encing great differences in heat and cold, does not suffer such ex- 
tremes of either as do the equatorial and tropical regions on the one 
hand, and the polar and subpolar on the other. Variations of climate 
are of two sorts: those associated with seasonality, and those asso- 
ciated with the succession of cyclones and anticyclones that is a 
distinguishing feature of these regions. Seasonality allows, as just 
implied, wide differences of temperature, yet provides against un- 
duly protracted periods of either heat or cold. It also makes possible 
interseasonal periods of moderate temperature. The succession of 
cyclones and anticyclones is not only the means by which this belt 
is provided throughout the year with moderate precipitation inter- 
spersed with intervals of clear weather, but is also the agency through 
which there proceeds an unceasing daily, and even hourly, variation 
in temperature, humidity, atmospheric pressure, and cloudiness. 

The beneficial influence of these conditions upon agricultural 
production is so obvious as to require no detailed explication. 

The influence of these climatic conditions upon human health 
and energy is less easily perceived. It is, of course, self-evident that 
the tropical, equatorial, and polar climates are less healthful than 
the temperate. The protracted cold and darkness of the arctic cli- 
mate are hostile to human life, and the almost complete absence of 
vegetable life and of domestic animals forces upon the arctic dweller 
a scanty and badly balanced diet. On the other hand, in the equa- 



POPULATION AND THE IMMIGRATION POLICY OF U.S. 101 

torial and tropical regions, where bacterial and insect life, abound, 
the human system is liable to attack by any one of a dozen deadly 
diseases. Again, the protracted heat and the excessive humidity of 
the equatorial rain-forest and tropical jungle are powerful depres- 
sants to the vitality, while the foods to which man most frequently 
must resort are so lacking in protein and so swelled with water as to 
leave the body deficiently nourished and the digestive system seri- 
ously impaired. 

Climate has, however, more subtle and far-reaching influences 
upon human life than those denoted by the presence or absence of 
positively deleterious elements, and it is to these less tangible factors 
that certain portions of the so-called temperate zone owe much of 
their climatic pre-eminence. As the investigations of Huntington 
and others have shown, there are certain optima of temperature and 
humidity that are most favorable to the maximum expenditure of 
human energy. For example, the optimum winter temperature is 
about 40, and the optimum summer temperature about 64. Refer- 
ence to the temperature map shows that, in January, the 40 iso- 
therm, and, in July, the 60 isotherm, run through the central por- 
tion of the United States, pass through West-Central Europe, and 
cross the northern portion of the Japanese archipelago. Again, mod- 
erate humidity such as these regions enjoy is favorable to nerve tone. 
Finally, the variability in these regions greatly increase energy po- 
tentiality. The cold of winter favors mental activity; the warmth of 
summer favors physical activity; and the moderate temperature of 
spring and fall promotes an all-around heightening of vitality. Again, 
the daily and hourly changes of temperature, cloudiness, and the 
like, have a powerful tonic effect on physical and mental energy. In 
sum, the regions of optimum climatic endowment not only make 
possible the largest yields of the best sort of agricultural products, 
but they also provide man with a capital of health and energy suf- 
ficient to enable him to make the most of the agricultural and other 
resources open to him. 

One further observation must be made concerning these regions 
of optimum endowment. They are fairly restricted in area. The 
Southern Hemisphere in general is very badly off in this respect. 
Africa is for the most part athwart the equator or the dry sub tropics. 



102 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

South America bulges in its equatorial belt and tapers sharply to- 
ward its temperate belt. Most of Australia is either equatorial rain- 
forest or subtropical desert. Again, even in the so-called north tem- 
perate zone, there are extensive mountain and continental desert 
areas both in the United States and in Asia, particularly in the 
latter, the bulk of the interior of which is deficient either in food- 
yielding or energy-producing qualities. There are left as really well- 
endowed countries only the regions named above, namely, most of the 
United States, Southern Canada, West-Central Europe, Japan, New 
Zealand, and fractional portions of South American and Australia. 

The extent of the favorable endowment of these regions may be 
inferred from a rapid survey of their economic productivity. The 
production maps for wheat and cattle all reveal a striking concen- 
tration of production in these regions. More remarkable is the com- 
posite map for wheat, maize (or corn), and rice, which allows for the 
variations in agricultural methods and dietary standards in different 
regions of the world. Turning from maps to tables, one obtains 
much the same impression as to the superior productivity of these 
regions. Thus in 1909-13, the nine regions United States, Russia 
in Europe, Canada, Hungary, Argentina, Germany, Roumania, Aus- 
tria, and the British Isles, all of them included in the belt of optimum 
climatic endowment produced 2,308,242,000 out of a world-total 
of 3,705,604,000 bushels of wheat, or about 62 per cent of the world- 
crop. In passing, it might be observed that wheat is not native to 
this belt, but originated in the semi-arid and subtropical regions; its 
extensive cultivation in the regions under discussion being an evi- 
dence of their superior crop-producing capacities. The world potato- 
crop is a yet clearer example of this tendency. Out of an average 
world-crop for the years 1909-13, of 5,407,033,000 bushels, these 
same nine countries together with France raised 4,449,839,000 bush- 
els, or 83 per cent of the total. 

Mineral production shows even greater concentration. For ex- 
ample, in 1920, the six regions United States, United Kingdom, 
Germany, Japan, France, and Belgium raised 1,160,235,509 metric 
tons of coal and lignite out of a world-production of 1,319,100,000 
metric tons; that is, 87 per cent of the total. The accident of location 
of the coal deposits has had something to do with this concentration, 
but the superior energy available for the mining of the coal in these 



POPULATION AND THE IMMIGRATION POLICY OF U.S. 103 

regions, and, above all, the more advanced civilization arising from 
this superior energy and from their superior agricultural productivity 
have probably had an even greater influence. In this connection, it 
may be observed that Huntington and Gushing point out that, al- 
though iron ore is one of the most widely distributed of all mineral 
deposits, yet the actual production of iron is confined almost entirely 
to the United States and to West-Central Europe. 

Since an abundant population is a natural consequence of high 
agricultural productivity and advanced industrialization, these same 
regions of optimum climatic endowment are among the most densely 
populated in the world. Thus, the three continents containing the 
three best favored regions, namely, Europe, Asia, and North Ameri- 
ca, lead in population density, albeit North America, for reasons to 
be discussed later, trails as a very poor third. It is within the con- 
tinents, however, that the pre-eminent population density of the 
better-favored regions is most strikingly revealed. In Europe, with 
an average density of 45.0 per square kilometer, thirteen of the fif- 
teen regions having a density of more than 60.0 per square kilometer, 
lie squarely in the belt of optimum climatic endowment. Of the two 
remaining, one, Portugal, is on the edge of this belt, while the other, 
Italy, has its northern portion well within it. 

In Asia the situation is not quite so well defined. Of the four 
regions with a density of over 60.0 per square kilometer, only one, 
namely, Japan, is clearly within the optimum belt. The other three 
are in the monsoon area where, particularly under the leadership of 
governing classes from the more energetic peoples of West-Central 
Europe, considerable population density may be achieved, although 
the quality of agricultural products raised and the climatic conditions 
yield insufficient health and energy for a progressive civilization. 
Moreover, it is to be noted that, excepting for the semi-urbanized and 
territorially insignificant Straits Settlements, none of these monsoon 
regions can compare in population density with the better-endowed 
West-Central European regions. In Oceania, leaving out of account 
islands of less than 50,000 square kilometers, New Zealand shows 
the greatest density. In Central and South America and Africa ter- 
ritorial divisions are so unequal in size and so arbitrary in nature 
that detailed comparisons are valueless. 

North America shows the same tendencies as Europe, the United 



104 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

States having a density of 13.5 per square kilometer, as against 7.0 
for Mexico and 0.9 for Canada. Furthermore, if the arid and semi- 
arid mountain states are left out of account, the density of the 
United States rises to 18.8. Moreover, within the United States it 
is the better-endowed Middle and South Atlantic and North-Central 
states which show the highest density, and this despite the priority 
in settlement and early development enjoyed by certain of the New 
England states. 

Of far greater significance, however, than the correspondence be- 
tween population density and climatic endowment in the United 
States is the sparsity of its population as compared with similarly 
endowed portions of Europe and Asia. The absolute density of the 
United States, leaving out of account the mountain states, is smaller 
than all of Europe and all of Asia, the figures being 18.8, 45.0, and 
24.2 per square kilometer, respectively, and this in spite of the fact 
that Europe contains extensive subpolar areas and Asia both sup- 
polar and desert areas. An even greater discrepancy appears when 
the United States is compared with the fifteen amost densely popu- 
lated regions of Europe whose climatic endowment is most similar to 
that of this country. Their average is 104.9 P er square kilometer, 
while that for the United States, less the mountain states, is 18.8. 
Again, if the five most densely populated areas of Europe, Asia, and 
America are compared, Europe is seen to range from 251.4 for Bel- 
gium to 124.2 for Italy; Asia, from 213.2 for Straits Settlements to 
38.6 for China; but the United States, only from 186.9 f r Rhode 
Island to 74.6 for Pennsylvania. Let it be repeated, the United 
States is one of the best-endowed regions, climatically reckoned, in 
the world. It is, in all probability, capable of supporting as many 
persons per square kilometer as Germany or Switzerland, or Hun- 
gary, if not England or Belgium or Holland, and is almost certainly 
capable of supporting a denser population than Japan. But its actual 
density, not counting the arid and semi-arid states, is less than that 
of European Russia, and only slightly more than that of Siam. Even 
when the ten most densely populated states of the United States are 
taken together, their aggregate density comes only to 51.1 per square 
kilometei, which is less than that of Lithuania and only slightly 
above that of Bulgaria, and, while higher than that of China, is still 



POPULATION AND THE IMMIGRATION POLICY OF U.S. 105 

below that of India. That is to say, the absolute population density 
of the United States is very much lower than that of equally, or even 
much less well, endowed regions in Europe and Asia. Putting the 
situation in terms of relative density, it would seem that, if the 
relative density of West-Central Europe were taken as 100, the rela- 
tive density of the United States would be somewhere between 25 
and 50. As stated above, it is impossible to assign exact magnitudes 
to relative population density with any accuracy. Nevertheless, it 
appears certain that, compared with West-Central Europe and 
Southeastern Asia, the relative density of population in the United 
States is low; and it is more than probable that compared with the 
remainder of Europe and Asia it is also low. 1 

This survey of the comparative population density of the United 
States, Europe, and Asia, may, therefore, be summarized as follows: 
Although possessing a climatic endowment ranking with the two or 
three best in the world, and, therefore, capable of supporting one of 
the densest populations in the world, the United States has a popula- 
tion density that is only moderate and that is very much below that 
of similarly endowed countries. 

It might be objected here that undue attention has been paid to 
climatic endowment as compared with soil fertility, mineral wealth, 
and the like. To this it may be replied that, considering the agricul- 
tural and mineral productivity of this country during the past hun- 
dred years, there can be little doubt but that it is at least as well 
endowed in these respects as the regions with which it is being com- 
pared; and if, as is likely, it is better endowed in some of these re- 
spects, the discrepancy between its potential and actual population- 
density is even greater than has been indicated. 

The effect of this situation upon the population equilibrium of 
the world may be readily perceived. It may be taken as a general 
principle that population tends to flow from regions of relatively 
high population-density to those of relatively low population-den- 
sity. It may be taken as a second general principle that, so long as 
any considerable area of the earth remains in which the relative 

1 Pearl assigns about 200,000,000 as the upper limit of American population growth. 
This would agree roughly with the estimate of 50 for its present density, made in the 
text. Cf. East, Mankind at the Crossroads : p. 151. 



io6 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

population density is low, the population equilibrium of the world 
will be unstable, and will remain unstable until migration from the 
relatively more densely populated regions shall have brought its 
density up to a level close to, if not equal with, their own. In more 
concrete terms, the original settlement of this country and the sub- 
sequent immigration to it are largely explicable as the response of 
the populations of Europe and to a lesser degree of Asia to the 
unstable population equilibrium growing out of the relatively low 
population-density of this country. And in the future this tendency 
must continue; the people of Europe and Asia must continue to de- 
sire and to seek homes in the United States until it is about as thickly 
settled as the similarly circumstanced countries in the older conti- 
nents. 

Nor must it be forgotten that the United States is the largest 
single factor in the present unstabilized condition of the world- 
population. There are plenty of regions in South America, Africa, 
or even Asia where the absolute population density is low, but it is to 
be doubted if their relative density is lower than, or even as low as, 
that of the United States. True, Southern South America, New 
Zealand, and Southeastern Australia are probably relatively as 
sparsely populated as this country, but their area is very restricted; 
Southern Canada, which is also of high potential and low actual 
density, is greater in extent, and is, in fact, practically a part of the 
United States population area. The United States and the adjoining 
Canadian provinces constitute, therefore, the area toward which the 
bulk of migration pressure is directed. 

Brief reference may be made here to the alleged dominance of 
economic motives in immigration to the United States. It has long 
been asserted that the higher wages, better standards of living, more 
rapid industrial expansion, and in an earlier generation greater 
amount of available land in this country than in Europe and Asia 
have been the leading factors in American immigration. All this is 
true enough, but these factors themselves are only the reflection of 
a low relative population-density. Easily available land depends 
directly on this condition; high wages and standards of living depend 
upon the high margin of cultivation due to this condition; and rapid 
industrial expansion is largely the result of the exploitation of abund- 
ant natural resources by a relatively sparse population. In short, 



POPULATION AND THE IMMIGRATION POLICY OF U.S. 107 

America has offered favorable economic opportunities to prospective 
settlers simply because it is relatively less crowded than the countries 
from which these settlers have migrated. 

The reaction of the new immigration policy of the United States 
upon the populations of these more densely peopled countries may 
now be considered. A restrictive immigration policy opposes a fun- 
damental and irrepressible one might almost say, irresistible ten- 
dency on the part of these populations to improve their condition by 
migrating to this, the outstanding region of high natural endowment 
and relatively low population-density remaining in the world. If 
this policy is long maintained, it is bound to react unfavorably upon 
the economic and social welfare of these peoples, and to arouse their 
resentment. 

One further point must be noted in this connection. So long as 
European immigration was merely regulated and not restricted, a 
policy of drastic restriction of Oriental immigration could be carried 
on without much friction. The country was rapidly being filled up, 
and the world population equilibrium was in the way of being 
brought into balance; only the process was being carried on by one 
rather than another set of peoples. Now, however, that the policy 
of Oriental exclusion has been extended to Japan at the same time 
that severe restriction has been imposed upon Europe, the friction 
resulting from Oriental exclusion may be expected to increase. That 
is, the United States has put up the bars against all of Europe and 
Asia, albeit the bars are higher toward Asia than toward Europe; 
and all of Europe and Asia, in so far as they are relatively more 
densely populated than the United States, have a common grievance 
against it. The United States is, in effect, fencing off a portion of 
the earth's surface and one of the fairest portions and is saying 
to the rest of the world that it intends to maintain indefinitely the 
favored economic and social conditions which its inhabitants enjoy 
because of their relatively low population-density, the rest of the 
world getting along as best it may, so long as its inhabitants keep 
away from the United States or arrive in just such numbers as the 
United States may choose. Legally and ethically, the United States 
may or may not find justification to adopt such a policy. The fact 
remains that such a policy adversely aflects the vital interests of 
millions of people in the relatively more densely populated portions 



io8 TEE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

of the globe, that those people are bound to resent it, and that they 
are more than likely to take measures to break it down. 

Are there any other courses of events that may ensue than the 
one just outlined? There are two, though neither appears to be 
within the limits of present possibility. First, science might devise 
ways of rendering the less-endowed regions capable of supporting 
a fairly dense population, and so converting them to areas of high 
potential and low actual density. If such a transformation were to 
take place, a part, at least, of the pressure of migration would move 
toward these regions and away from the United States. Yet science 
must not only improve the healthfulness of these regions, but must 
also increase their food-raising and energy-yielding capacities to 
bring this about, and there is no immediate prospect that this three- 
fold task is near accomplishment. 

In the second place, the birth-rate of this country might adjust 
itself to the slackened immigration, and so increase as to cause the 
country to be filled up, and the unstable population equilibrium re- 
moved about as rapidly as if no restriction had taken place, the only 
difference being that the increasing population density would come 
almost altogether by excess of births over deaths, and not merely 
in part from this cause, as at present. In the long run, this might 
very well take place, always provided that artificial limitation of 
families does not become so widespread as to upset the normal 
tendencies of population growth. But, on the other hand, before 
this change could come about, the reactions to the American immi- 
gration policy might well have assumed serious proportions. Also, it 
must be remembered that the excess of births over deaths is showing 
no diminution, to say the least, in Asia, whose absolute density 
increased by 3.5 per square kilometer in the decade 1910-20. 

It goes without saying that a modification of the new immigra- 
tion policy of the United States, or even the failure to apply it to 
migrants from the Western Hemisphere, would once more release 
the forces tending to restore the population equilibrium of the world, 
and so resolve the conflict of interests discussed in this paper. 

NOTE. This paper in slightly expanded and altered form, together with 
the maps, charts, and tables shown on the stereopticon screen during its original 
reading, have been printed by the University of Buffalo, and may be obtained 
by application to the Dean of the College of Arts and Sciences. 



THE NEED FOR IMPROVED CHILD-WELFARE 
STATISTICS 



ROBERT M. WOODBURY 
Institute of Economics, Washington, D.C. 



ABSTRACT 

The progress of child welfare depends upon adequate statistics with which to define 
problems, analyze causes, and test results. Complete birth statistics are needed to throw 
light upon the growth of population, the problems connected with illegitimate births, 
the quality of confinement care, and upon the conditions which affect infant mortality. 
Data are needed to show pathological and underlying causes of deaths in infancy and 
early childhood, and to indicate progress in prevention not only of mortality but also of 
morbidity and of physical defects. Statistics of accidents to children in the home, on the 
streets, and in industry are needed for use in guiding prevention. Data on the extent of 
delinquency and dependency, on their causes, and on methods of care of dependent and 
delinquent children are needed for an understanding of these social problems and of the 
changes which are taking place in them. Statistics of the prevalence of child labor and 
of the evils which it causes are needed to show the urgency of measures for its control. 



The past fifteen years have seen a great improvement in child- 
welfare statistics. This improvement has not been confined to any 
one phase of child welfare, but characterizes to a greater or lesser 
degree all aspects of the subject. The present paper is designed to 
take account of stock, as it were, of these statistics. Are our present 
statistical resources adequate, what gaps are there which need to be 
filled, and in what directions do we need more data? 

Fundamentally, we need such resources not for their own sake 
but for use in the solution of our social problems. What are the 
principal problems of child welfare in regard to which we need statis- 
tical data? What data do we need in order to be able to control these 
problems? These are the fundamental questions to be answered; and 
in the light of their answers we must determine whether our present 
statistics are adequate, and how they should be developed so as to 
approach more closely to our requirements. 

The problems to the solution of which statistics should be directed 
concern the welfare of some forty million children under eighteen 
years of age, taking a fairly broad definition, in the United States. 
Any complete consideration of all such problems would far exceed 

109 



no THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

the limits of a single paper. We shall select, therefore, for discussion 
certain aspects of the statistics relating to a few outstanding topics: 
births, mortality, health and physical condition, accidents, defects, 
dependency, delinquency, and child labor. We shall omit entirely 
from consideration education, physical, mental, and moral growth 
and development, family statistics, mothers' pensions, and many 
other specific problems as to which adequate statistics are equally 
necessary. 

Problems which are directly concerned with the number and dis- 
tribution of births include those associated with the decline in the 
birth-rate, its causes and consequences, with differential birth-rates 
in different social and economic classes, with size of family, with the 
eugenic development of the population, and many others. Child 
welfare is closely bound up, for example, with the economic condi- 
tion of the families into which children are born, and with the num- 
ber of children in the family. The circumstances surrounding births 
may give rise to problems which vitally affect the welfare of children. 
Whether a child is legitimate or illegitimate affects not only his 
chances of happiness but also his chances of life. The problems of 
survival or death of children are among the most important for 
which statistics of births are needed. 

With regard to this subject it will be agreed that we ought, first 
of all, to know the number of births that occur each year. We do not 
know this number exactly, though we can estimate it roughly at 
2,500,000. Such an estimate is based upon records for the birth- 
registration area, which comprises states in which registration is 
accepted by the Bureau of the Census as at least 90 per cent com- 
plete. This area now includes thirty-two states and the District of 
Columbia, and three-fourths of the population of the country. 

Remarkable progress has been made in statistics of births during 
the past ten years. Beginning with the year 1915, the birth-registra- 
tion area has grown from ten states and the District of Columbia 
with 31 per cent of the population in 1915 to thirty- two states and 
the District of Columbia with 75 per cent of the population in 1924. 

But even today we are ignorant of the number of births in six- 
teen states, comprising one-fourth of the population, for which regis- 
tration fails to come up to the standards set for admission to the 



NEED FOR IMPROVED CHILD-WELFARE STATISTICS in 

area. We can be satisfied with nothing less than complete birth 
registration. It is a disgrace that a nation which considers itself 
civilized should have such defective records of births. 

Not only do we need to extend the birth-registration area, but 
even within the area we need more complete and accurate statistics. 
The acceptance of a state by the Bureau of the Census does not 
mean that the data are complete, but merely that the omissions 
are judged, on the basis of more or less satisfactory tests made at 
the time of admission and in some cases also later, to be less than 
10 per cent. At the present stage of development we need data to 
show the accuracy of these statistics, to test the proportion of omis- 
sions. These tests should be made annually. A comparison of birth 
registration in 1919 with the results of the census enumeration of 
population under one year of age at the close of that year, in con- 
junction with statistics of infant deaths and with a special test made 
in the District of Columbia, leads to the conclusion that in 1919 the 
omissions of births averaged 8 per cent for the entire registration 
area, and in the different states varied, so far as could be judged by 
the evidence at hand, from nearly zero to 19 per cent. 

Such tests are needed to check up and to correct conclusions 
which may be based upon these statistics, for example, conclusions 
as to the decline in the birth-rate, as to relative rates among white 
and colored, as to relative rates in different sections of the country, 
and as to relative rates of infant mortality. In 1920, for example, the 
birth-rates for the colored population, compared state for state with 
those for the white population, indicated that the rates for colored 
were, on the average, below those forwhites. Was this due simply to a 
larger percentage of omissions or of errors affecting the statistics of 
colored births? Birth registration, in general, probably improves 
from year to year. Since registered births are used, for example, in 
calculating infant-mortality rates and since an improvement in regis- 
tration appears in an apparent decrease in these rates, it is important 
to know how great this improvement is in order to correct the appar- 
ent trend in the rates of infant mortality. The improvement of 
registration in the birth-registration states of 1915 during the period 
from 1915 to 1921 may be estimated, for example, at 2.9 per cent. 

Three items on which information is now available from birth 



H2 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

records but which have not hitherto been tabulated on any compre- 
hensive scale offer possibilities for immediate development. Whether 
or not the birth occurred in a hospital is a question the answer to 
which is contained on the birth certificates, but no statistics on this 
point are available except for a few localities. In some cities for 
which the data have been tabulated, for example in Minneapolis, 
the statistics show a marked change during recent years in the pro- 
portion of births in hospitals. During this period a decided improve- 
ment has taken place in hospital facilities for confinement cases. The 
statistics throw light, therefore, upon a very significant change af- 
fecting the quality of confinement care, which directly influences the 
health and chances of life of children as well as of the mothers. 

A second question which bears upon the same problem is whether 
the birth was attended by a physician. On this point, also, the avail- 
able data have been tabulated for a few localities and states; they 
show interesting contrasts between different races and nationalities 
and between different sections of the country; over a period of years, 
in certain communities, important changes have taken place in cus- 
toms regarding attendance at birth. Such data ought to be made 
available for the entire country in order that the nature of the prob- 
lem, its local variations, and the changes taking place in it may be 
more fully appreciated. 

Other items needed for statistical purposes should be added to 
the original records. Among these may be mentioned that of whether 
the child was born prematurely an item which throws light upon 
the physical condition at birth and the interval of time since the 
preceding birth or, in case of a first birth, the interval since marriage. 
Information on this latter point would be invaluable in a study of 
the spacing of births, which appears to have an important effect on 
infant mortality, and statistics of the spacing of births would be of 
great interest also as an indication of the prevalence of voluntary 
control over the birth-rate. 

The high mortality during the first year of life has been and still 
is one of the major problems of child welfare. In 1915, according to 
the statistics for the birth-registration area, 10 per cent, or one hun- 
dred out of every thousand of all children born died under one year 



NEED FOR IMPROVED CHILD-WELFARE STATISTICS 113 

of age. Although this proportion has been greatly reduced, the rate 
is still 76 per 1000. In an estimate of the future prolongation of life 
which might be achieved by making universal the results which have 
been obtained in some localities, Dr. Dublin uses the figure of 38 per 
1000 as an attainable infant-mortality rate. This figure is exactly 
one-half the rate of the birth-registration area in 1922. In passing, 
it may be noted that New Zealand's rate in that year was 42, only 
four points higher than Dr. Dublin's estimate. 

Death statistics in this country are somewhat better developed 
than birth statistics, although in comparison with the mortality 
statistics of other countries they must be regarded as backward. 
Eighty-two per cent of the population is included in the death- 
registration area, for which annual statistics are published. But so 
far as infant mortality is concerned the statistics are limited, in 
effect, to the area for which adequate birth statistics are available, 
since to calculate infant-mortality rates deaths under one year of 
age must be related to births. 

As in the case of birth statistics, we need complete and accurate 
data on mortality if we are properly to appreciate our problems. 
Statistics showing the rates of mortality in different localities and in 
different nationalities indicate where the problem is most acute and 
hence where preventive measures are most needed. Data showing 
the causes of infant mortality and the conditions and circumstances 
which accompany high and low rates are also invaluable as showing 
the specific evils which methods of prevention should be designed to 
attack, and to study the effect of preventive measures. 

Possibilities of immediate development are offered by two items 
which are at present available on the original death-records but 
which have hitherto not been utilized. We ought to have informa- 
tion in regard to the quality of certification of the causes of death 
whether by a physician or by some other person. Probably in most 
cases the certificate is signed by a physician, but we ought to know 
not only the proportion of cases in which it is signed by someone 
else, but also a point not now included in the records whether a 
physician was in attendance prior to the death. This information 
would assist us in appraising the statistics of causes and at the 



H4 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

same time would give us valuable data on the medical care of young 
children. 

A second point which should be tabulated is the year of birth. If 
death statistics were analyzed both as to age at death and as to the 
calendar year of birth, it would be possible to follow in the statistics 
each annual cohort of births through life, and thus to obtain accurate 
probabilities of dying at each year of age. Any other method is sub- 
ject to errors arising from fluctuations in the number of births occur- 
ring in each year. 

Much valuable data on the conditions which produce infant 
mortality could be obtained by linking up information on birth cer- 
tificates with that on death certificates of infants under one year of 
age, either by matching the records and transferring the data from 
one to the other prior to tabulation or by providing spaces for original 
entries on the death certificate in the same form as those on the birth 
certificate. Among such items may be mentioned legitimacy, inter- 
val since preceding birth, order of birth, single or plural birth, pre- 
maturity, data relating to the physical condition and care of the 
mother before the confinement, and data in regard to the economic 
environment into which the child was born. 

In regard to legitimacy, for example, the statistics of mortality 
would yield data bearing upon the influence of conditions to which 
illegitimate children are exposed in contrast to those which surround 
legitimate children. The mortality rate forms, as it were, a criterion 
by which to test and judge these conditions. With accurate data on 
this subject and there are difficulties, it will be agreed, in obtaining 
such data it would become possible to trace the influence of pre- 
ventive measures adopted in order to better the lot of the child 
born out of wedlock. The United States is one of the few countries 
without comprehensive statistics as to the mortality of illegitimate 
children. 

Special statistical studies, such as those made by the Children's 
Bureau, of the influences which affect the chances of life of illegiti- 
mate children, are, of course, also necessary. The separation of the 
child from his mother, for example, is, in case of illegitimate children, 
a very important factor in infant mortality among them. 

In connection with the subject of infant mortality, the need for 



NEED FOR IMPROVED CHILD-WELFARE STATISTICS 115 

statistics showing the scope and effect of preventive work should be 
emphasized. It should be possible to measure, by means of statistics 
of the changing proportions of children artificially fed, the results 
obtained through educational work directed toward decreasing the 
prevalence of artificial feeding. We need statistics showing the pro- 
portion and the ages of children who are reached by infant-welfare 
centers in order to test whether such centers are adequately meeting 
the situation. We need to know what proportion of children are 
actually brought under the influence of the measures designed to 
protect them; what proportions of mothers receive adequate care 
before and at birth; what proportion of mothers receive instruction 
in child care. Data on these points would prove of great value in 
evaluating the work already under way and in stimulating to greater 
efforts. 

The problems of mortality among older children are less serious 
than those of mortality among children under one year. But we 
need statistics in regard to the extent and causes of these deaths. We 
ought to know what proportion of them are preventable and how 
they can be prevented. The available data indicate the great im- 
portance during this age period of communicable diseases as causes 
of death. They suggest also the importance of "external causes/' 
accidents, as a factor in mortality. This subject leads, therefore, 
directly to the consideration of statistics of morbidity and of acci- 
dents. 

The problems which arise in connection with morbidity can be 
stated very simply. We ought to know the extent of disease and its 
consequences, and the character and extent of preventive work and 
its effectiveness. In the first place, we need data to show the inci- 
dence of each of the important contagious diseases and the fatality 
rates at different ages. One source of such statistics is the compul- 
sory reports of these diseases made to local health officers; unfortu- 
nately these records are, in most places, far from complete. Another 
source of information concerning illness from these and other causes 
is the records of absences from school in conjunction with the records 
of medical inspections of school children; the United States Public 
Health Service has in progress an extensive study of morbidity 
among school children in Hagerstown, Maryland. Another possible 



n6 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

source is a sickness census; the scope of such a census, however, is 
distinctly limited. Hospital records also offer possibilities for study. 
The administrative statistician who attempts to obtain accurate 
data on these subjects, it will be conceded, has some knotty prob- 
lems to solve. Fortunately, in testing preventive methods reliance 
need not be placed wholly upon statistics of sickness, since changes 
in prevalence or in the fatality of diseases are reflected also in statis- 
tics of mortality. 

Of no less importance are statistics relating to the scope and 
effectiveness of methods of prevention. We need information in re- 
gard to vaccination; unfortunately within recent years less and less 
attention appears to have been paid to vaccinating children against 
smallpox, with the result that a recent press release of the Public 
Health Service asserted that the United States ranks third among 
the principal countries in prevalence of this serious disease. We need 
data, not only as to the proportion of children who are vaccinated, 
but also as to the ill effects of vaccination, if any, and new data con- 
cerning its effectiveness, in order that there may be available for the 
doubters fresh and convincing evidence. In the new methods for 
preventing diphtheria, we need data on the number and proportion 
of children who are subjected to the S chick test and to the immun- 
ization procedure, in order that the possible results of the general 
introduction of these measures may be properly evaluated. 

We need to know more than we do about the physical quality of 
our child population. But the statistician who desires to assess the 
physical fitness of children has serious difficulties to overcome; for 
example, difficulties in establishing uniform standards of diagnosis. 
It may prove impossible to obtain such data accurately. Yet even 
data subject to a considerable margin of error, such as the draft 
records of men called to service, or the records of physical defects 
found in children under six years of age who were weighed and meas- 
ured and whose defects were noted by physicians during the "Chil- 
dren's Year" campaign may prove invaluable in awakening the pub- 
lic mind to the need for remedial work. Except for such general 
surveys as may be feasible in connection with some such campaign, 
statistics of minor physical defects have to be based largely upon 
the records of school physicians. Records of physical examinations 



NEED FOR IMPROVED CHILD-WELFARE STATISTICS 117 

made, in some states, as a preliminary to granting employment cer- 
tificates, if standardized and if not perfunctory, might also be util- 
ized for statistical purposes. 

Statistics relating to the remedial measures taken for these defects 
would be valuable as indicating the scope of preventive measures. 
Among such defects are defective teeth. A very large proportion of 
children of school ages it is estimated as high as 95 per cent have 
one or more decayed teeth. We need data showing not only the 
prevalence of these defects but also to what extent they are being 
remedied, and how adequate or inadequate are our resources for 
remedying them; in other words, statistics concerning their correc- 
tion. 

Passing over the broad fields of normal growth and development, 
physical, mental, moral, including such data as those relating to 
heights and weights at different ages, we turn to the statistics of 
accidents. We need statistics of accidents, fatal and non-fatal, ac- 
cidents at home, in the streets, and in industrial establishments. We 
have a fairly complete record of deaths from accidents in the regis- 
tration states; it shows that about one- third of all fatal accidents 
occur to children. But only fragmentary data are available in regard 
to non-fatal accidents, such as those for New York City, for ex- 
ample, where over ten thousand accidents to children under sixteen 
are reported to the police each year. There is a general impression, 
gained from newspaper reports, that a large number of children are 
killed and injured in traffic accidents. We need to know more about 
the causes of these, and about why the children were in the streets 
where they could be injured; whether, for example, it was because of 
lack of playgrounds. We have almost no data on home accidents 
except such as relate to deaths from poison, from burns, and from 
other causes. 

Industrial-accident statistics for the most part do not distinguish 
ages, so that we are practically without information as to whether 
in industry in general the risk of accident is greater or less to child 
than to adult workers in the same occupations. In regard to these 
accidents information is needed in order to provide a statistical basis 
for protective legislation. Accidents to children, if found to be par- 
ticularly serious, may be prevented by limiting the ages at which 



n8 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

children may be employed, and by restricting their occupations in 
such a way that work on dangerous machinery or in dangerous 
processes is prohibited. Such legislation should, of course, be based 
upon an analysis of the statistics of industrial accidents. 

Many social and economic problems arise in connection with the 
care of defective, dependent, and delinquent children. In regard to 
these classes we ought to know how numerous they are, how they 
are cared for, and whether the care provided is adequate and satis- 
factory. 

With regard to the so-called dependent and neglected children, 
the principal problem is that of insuring that they do not suffer in 
their physical, mental, or moral development because of the handi- 
caps to which the breakdown of family responsibilities on the part 
of the father, mother, or both, has exposed them. We need statistical 
evidence to show the number of such children, the reasons why they 
are dependent or neglected, and the methods of care. More and 
more the child-care societies are turning away from the idea of plac- 
ing children in orphanages or other institutions, and are placing them 
in carefully selected and supervised family homes. 

The need for more adequate statistics of dependent children is 
illustrated by our very ignorance of the magnitude of the changes in 
social policy which are being carried out. We need statistics to show 
accurately the scope of these changes in methods and the advantages 
and disadvantages of each mode of care. Some interesting researches 
have been made, for example, into the relative physical development 
of children living in institutions as compared with that of children 
living in family homes. A comprehensive study to show the relation 
between the type of care and the physical, mental, and moral de- 
velopment of dependent children would be a most valuable addition 
to our statistics of this subject. 

A hopeful development in the statistics of dependent children is 
the adoption by a number of state boards of control of a system of 
compulsory reporting of children received in institutions or by child- 
care societies. Such a system, if successfully developed, would result 
in a continuous registration record of all dependent children in the 
state. Such records should furnish invaluable data, not only in re- 
gard to the numbers of such children, but also in regard to many 



NEED FOR IMPROVED CHILD-WELFARE STATISTICS 119 

other points age, cause of dependency, condition of family from 
which received, type of care, and type of placement. Information in 
regard to the ages at which children are brought to the attention of 
an agency or institution, in conjunction with whether the cases are 
first or later applications, would furnish evidence concerning the 
incidence of dependency. 

Statistics are also needed to show the history of dependent chil- 
dren over a period of years. What becomes of them? What influence, 
if any, do the conditions of dependency exert over their later develop- 
ment? In such studies the records of central boards would prove a 
valuable point of departure. 

The development of these records of central agencies in the differ- 
ent states suggests immediately the importance of having their sta- 
tistics analyzed and tabulated by a federal agency. In time a regis- 
tration area for such statistics might be established similar to the 
birth- and death-registration areas. In this way uniformity could be 
secured and the statistics would grow more and more comprehensive 
and would more and more adequately fill our needs. 

With regard to delinquency the need for statistics is obvious. 
How many juvenile delinquents are there? Are they mostly in cities 
or are they equally distributed between city and country? In what 
types of courts are their cases heard juvenile courts or other types? 
What kind of delinquencies bring children before the courts? What 
are the causes which tend to produce delinquencies? What kind of 
parental control do the parents of juvenile delinquents exercise? Are 
special conditions, such as divorce, unhappy family life, or the men- 
tal or moral unfitness of one or both parents, responsible for juvenile 
delinquency? 

Special emphasis should be laid upon the fact that the most im- 
portant point in connection with statistics of juvenile delinquency is 
not the number of cases but the number of children concerned. First 
offenses, therefore, should be carefully separated from all other of- 
fenses. Statistics of first offenses, if accurately compiled, show the 
number of different children concerned in juvenile delinquency, and 
furnish a basis for a sound conclusion as to whether it is increasing 
or decreasing. Such data should be analyzed to show the age in- 
cidence of juvenile delinquency. 



120 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

In the treatment and care of delinquent children, as in the case 
of dependent children, a great change in methods has been taking 
place during the past quarter of a century. More and more, especial- 
ly in the larger cities, children's cases are brought before special 
juvenile courts. Probation service has been developed. Placing of 
delinquent children in family homes is increasingly adopted as a 
method of caring for them. We need comprehensive statistics by 
which we may trace the progress of these developments, showing 
the proportion of cases which are treated according to approved 
methods. 

Furthermore, we need statistics by which the results of these 
various methods can be tested. Are they successful? What propor- 
tion of the juvenile delinquents whose cases are handled according 
to approved methods fail to become useful members of society? We 
need data based upon the histories of juvenile delinquents subse- 
quent to the first offense, and compiled in relation to the character 
of treatment which their cases received and the conditions which 
surrounded them. In this connection statistics of offenses other than 
first furnish evidence upon which, in part, the treatment of juvenile 
offenders may be judged. 

Statistics of juvenile prisoners and of children sentenced to re- 
formatories and other penal or correctional institutions have been 
collected in connection with censuses of the population of jails, pris- 
ons, and other similar institutions. We have also data with reference 
to the number of courts hearing children's cases, and an estimate 
of the number of cases heard. But we are greatly in need not only 
of comprehensive statistics of the annual volume of juvenile delin- 
quency, but also detailed studies of its causes and of the conditions 
which produce it. 

Many social and economic problems arise also in connection with 
child labor and its regulation. Perhaps the chief problem is that of 
the effect of early employment of children upon their educational 
opportunities. Other problems are concerned with the child's health 
and development, his liability to accident, and the effect of early 
labor upon his later industrial career. 

We need statistics, first of all, to show the extent of child labor 
at different ages and in different occupations. We have census sta- 



NEED FOR IMPROVED CHILD-WELFARE STATISTICS 121 

tistics on this subject showing by age groups the numbers of children 
from ten to twenty years of age reported to be engaged in gainful 
occupations. These statistics are probably subject to considerable 
margin of error, since the decision whether or not the child is em- 
ployed is left to the individual enumerator. The census bureau ig- 
nores any reports of employment of children under ten years of age. 
Intensive studies made by the Children's Bureau have indicated the 
extent of child labor in selected occupations and in selected localities. 
These studies have emphasized the close connection between early 
child labor and lack of education and have sought to trace its effects. 

Between intercensal periods statistics relating to the granting 
of employment certificates should be available for use in studying 
changes in the proportions of children employed. The systematic 
compilation of such certificates would be of great value, since they 
usually require a statement of the child's education and frequently 
of his physical condition. On the basis of these records, therefore, 
it should be possible to study the education and physical condition 
of children to whom employment certificates are granted. 

We also need statistics to show the extent to which vocational 
guidance is available for children entering industry. Statistics of 
placement are needed to show what occupations are open to children. 
Do children enter occupations which offer a definite industrial future, 
or do they enter blind-alley occupations, or during their early years 
in industry do they perform merely children's work? Are their early 
years in industry valuable or valueless for training purposes? 

We need to know more about the adaptation of children to in- 
dustrial conditions. How soon and how completely do children be- 
come adjusted to their industrial environment? How long do they 
hold their positions? What opportunities are open to those who are 
handicapped physically or mentally? These are but a few of the 
questions upon which more adequate statistical evidence is required. 

In conclusion, three points should be emphasized. In the first 
place, it is obviously impossible in a brief paper to make any com- 
plete statement of our needs for child-welfare statistics. Time re- 
strictions prevent a full development of the subjects touched upon, 
and many subjects have had to be left out altogether. Secondly, it 
has been quite impossible to give adequate attention to the history 



122 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

and development of such statistics, an interesting subject which is 
in need of comprehensive treatment. The third point follows nat- 
urally: that, since the forward-looking point of view seemed most 
important, the emphasis in the paper has been placed upon our needs 
rather than upon our accomplishments. Though this emphasis upon 
needs, in connection with the limitations of time, has precluded any 
adequate discussion of our achievements, they are none the less real 
and substantial, and in closing I wish to refer to the splendid pioneer 
work that has been accomplished in many lines of child-welfare 
statistics by the Bureau of the Census, the Children's Bureau, and 
other agencies which have devoted time and energy to the gathering 
of statistics with the aim of advancing the solution of the problems 
of child welfare. 



THE DEVELOPMENT OF AMERICAN VITAL 
STATISTICS 



WALTER F. WILLCOX 
Cornell University 



ABSTRACT 

The legal and administrative basis for a good system of registering births and 
deaths has been laid since 1900 in every American state except South Dakota, Arizona, 
and Nevada. To the results of this system is being added the nation-wide registration 
of marriages and divorces. Statistical details about marriages are almost entirely 
lacking. We may look forward, accordingly, to the completion in a very few years of a 
unique nation-wide system of vital statistics based on purely voluntary co-operation 
between the states and the federal government. Unfortunately the completeness of 
registration, especially in states newly added to the registration area and in the rural 
districts of many states earlier admitted, leaves much to be desired. In constructive 
criticism and interpretation of the vital statistics thus published scholars now have a 
large field for important and remunerative work. 



Vital statistics include those statistics which deal with the pro- 
cesses whereby society perpetuates itself notwithstanding the brief 
life of each member. They embrace the statistics of births and 
deaths, of marriages as the normal and usual antecedent of births, 
and, hi addition to the deaths of married persons, divorces and de- 
sertions as other ways of ending a marriage union. Before 1900 the 
United States ^tlone among civilized countries was without a national 
system of vital statistics and had no prospect of soon developing one. 
Since that date a unique system has been growing up by voluntary 
co-operation of the federal government with the states and cities. 

In 1900, American citizens interested in the public-health move- 
ment and in the statistics of deaths as a cornerstone, if not indeed a 
much larger part of the foundation, of that movement were much 
more numerous than those who cared about any other branch of 
vital statistics. Many states and cities acting independently had 
gone far toward building up good systems, many others had made 
valiant efforts toward the same end, but, especially when they relied 
upon the county which has proved to be an unworkable registration 
unit, they had almost completely failed. Thus the statistics of deaths 

123 



124 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

were indicated as the starting-point. The laws, ordinances, and sta- 
tistical forms used in any part of the United States for the registration 
of deaths were collected and compared. On this basis an outline plan 
suited to American conditions was drafted. It included the points 
that ought to be covered in any satisfactory law or ordinance and a 
model form of death certificate. The American Public Health As- 
sociation aided in the work and indorsed the result. At this stage the 
subject was laid before Congress together with a specimen form of 
law. Congress, at the suggestion of the Bureau of the Census, passed 
a joint resolution approving the effort and requesting the states to 
consider and act upon the subject. The great majority of the states 
which at the beginning of the century were registering deaths im- 
mediately adopted the new form of certificate and thus laid the basis 
for uniform federal statistics. The movement was indorsed a little 
later by the Conference of State and Provincial Boards of Health and 
the American Medical Association. It spread rapidly and its success 
soon aroused a demand for similar federal leadership in the matter of 
registering births. A model law and certificate for the registration 
of births were prepared by the same agencies and quite widely 
adopted. 

The states and cities, it will be noticed, have retained all their 
authority unimpaired, and the federal government with its occasion- 
al conferences, its steady stream of correspondence, and its expert 
secretariat has played somewhat the role of the League of Nations. 
The only power exercised by the federal government was that vested 
by Congress in the director of the census to collect, tabulate, and 
publish at its expense and in his discretion the statistics of deaths 
and births in the registration area; that is, those parts of the country 
in which deaths or births were registered with completeness enough 
to give value to the statistics. After a state had enacted a satisfac- 
tory law usually the model law had provided for its proper ad- 
ministration, and was getting at least 90 per cent of its deaths or 
births recorded, it was admitted to the registration area. The de- 
velopment of this area is shown in Table I. 

Only three states remain, South Dakota, Arizona, and Nevada, 
in which the law is unsatisfactory because it does not and cannot 
secure a good registration of births and deaths, and those three 



THE DEVELOPMENT OF AMERICAN VITAL STATISTICS 125 



states are among the most sparsely settled. In a number of other 
states, especially those in which the registration law is of recent 
enactment, it is the appropriation which is inadequate or lacking, or 
the administration which is ineffective. In seven states the registra- 
tion of deaths, and in fourteen the registration of births, are thus on 
trial. If the recent rate of progress should continue, all states would 
be in both registration areas by 1930, and then for the first time we 
would know the American birth-rate and death-rate at least ap- 
proximately as we know those of almost every other civilized 
country. 

Annual reports upon marriages and divorces in the United States 
began in 1922 and seem likely to be continued. These statistics also 
are gathered, wherever possible, from state offices. But many states 

TABLE I 



DATE 


NUMBER OF STATES ADMITTED 
TO REGISTRATION AREA FOR 


PERCENTAGE OF POPULATION 
INCLUDED IN REGISTRATION 
AREA FOR 


Deaths 


Births 


Deaths 


Births 


IQOO . . . 


II 

21 

25 
35 
39 




40-5 
58.3 
67-5 
82.2 

87.7 




IQIO 


ii 

25 
32 




IQIC 


3I.I 
59-8 
74-5 


IO2O 


IQ24. 





do not require returns of manages and divorces. In them the statis- 
tics are obtained by the Bureau of the Census directly from the coun- 
ties, either by correspondence or, in the few cases in which that fails, 
by special agents in the field. The statistics of marriages thus secured 
are confined to the total number registered. It would be appropriate, 
I believe, for organizations like the Sociological Society to urge that 
a model law for the registration of marriages like those for the regis- 
tration of births and deaths should be prepared and laid before the 
states. Indeed, if requested, it might co-operate in drafting such a 
law. Until such a uniform law for the registration of marriages, 
specifying the indispensable statistical details, is enacted in a number 
of states no adequate statistics of marriages can be obtained. If that 
were done the Bureau of the Census might continue its present work 
of gathering the mere number of marriages from the entire country, 



126 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

but in addition prepare statistics showing the age, race, birthplace, 
previous marital condition, etc., of the parties for those states in 
which the uniform registration law was in force, an area which might 
then be called the marriage registration area. 

Existing statistics about divorce are more detailed and uniform. 
The need for a model law for the registration of divorces, therefore, is 
less clear, but I believe that if one for marriages were widely adopted 
a demand for a uniform law for the registration of divorces would 
quickly arise, just as a demand for a birth-registration law followed 
upon the adoption of a death-registration law. 

The Bureau of Immigration now reports the number of depart- 
ing emigrants as well as the number of arriving immigrants. Prob- 
ably in the near future methods for registering immigrants entering 
from Canada or Mexico and for preventing clandestine immigration 
will be improved. By 1930, then, we may hope to possess the ele- 
ments of a complete system of vital statistics obtained in most cases 
by a unique form of voluntary co-operation between the federal 
government and the states. This progress should give much satis- 
faction to the sociologists of the country. They, as much as any 
organized body of men and women, represent the public which de- 
mands and uses the information thus furnished. It should gratify 
especially those who agree with me in deploring the tendency to 
increase federal power and functions at the expense of the states. 

The members of the Sociological Society will wish, also, to know 
something about the quality and significance of American vital sta- 
tistics. Let us turn, then, to the far more difficult topic, the com- 
pleteness and trustworthiness of these records and of the rates based 
upon them. I pass by the problems of estimating post-censal pop- 
ulation, upon which I have recently spoken before another assem- 
bly, 1 and that of the correctness with which causes of death are as- 
signed by the attending physicians or other persons charged by the 
law with that difficult and delicate task. The latter is of interest 
primarily to physicians, and is easily separable from the question I 
desire now to raise: What proportion of all deaths in the death- 
registration area are registered? I will limit it more narrowly by 

x International Mathematical Congress held at Toronto in August, 1924. See 
"Methods of Estimating the Population of the U.S.,' 1 in Metron for 1925. 



THE DEVELOPMENT OF AMERICAN VITAL STATISTICS 127 

phrasing it thus: What proportion of the deaths which occurred in 
the death-registration states in 1920, the year of the most recent 
census, were registered? 

In this field I am more skeptical than most students and my 
arguments should be weighed with a presumption against accepting 
them, because I speak for what is probably a small minority among 
those in touch with the situation. In no community on either side of 
the Atlantic, I believe, do all deaths get recorded. Perhaps the larg- 
est class of omissions is in the group of very young children who 
breathe for only a few days, hours, or even minutes, after birth. In 
some such cases no entry appears; in others the death is registered 
as a stillbirth, either contrary to the law which defines a stillborn 
child as one dead at birth, or in conformity with it in jurisdictions 
which allow a birth to be registered as a stillbirth if the child is dead 
before registration. Other classes among which omissions are likely 
to occur are the group of accidental deaths, for example by drowning, 
in which the body is not recovered. According to the theory of the 
American registration law, no corpse can be buried, cremated, or 
transported without a permit, and that permit is to be issued only 
in exchange for a certificate of death containing the required personal 
description of the deceased and a medical or verified statement of 
the cause of death. In surveying the evidence for my claim that 
many deaths in certain of the registration states are not registered, 
I shall be frankly personal, because that appeals to me as the most 
cogent and also the most interesting way of arguing. 

Nearly thirty years ago the citizens of a small city in central New 
York were faced by the question of whether they should instal a 
city sewerage system. Some of the taxpayers objected to the city's 
assuming the heavy cost, and urged that its low death-rate, 13,4 per 
1,000, proved the city to be an unusually healthy place, which had 
accordingly little need to spend money on the projected improv- 
ment. A careful study of the local situation carried on for weeks by 
several members of a university class in statistics proved that the 
number of unrecorded deaths in the year, 1896, was equal to one- 
third the number of recorded deaths, and that the true death-rate 
of the city was 16.5, instead of 13.4, per 1,000. This evidence may 
have had some influence on the voters; at any rate the plan for a 



128 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

sewerage system was adopted. In 1920 the death-rates of Ithaca, 
New York, Ann Arbor, Michigan, Bloomington, Indiana, and Colum- 
bia, Missouri, to speak only of university towns, were 15 per 1,000, 
or more, and thus not intrinsically improbable. But the rates in 
Urbana, Illinois: n.6, Madison, Wisconsin: 11.5, and Berkeley, 
California: 10.3, were so low as to arouse skepticism and to invite 
examination. One would like to see in them a careful test made like 
that described in Ithaca. Our best source for checking registration 
proved to be the deaths mentioned in the file of the local daily paper. 
If we turn to the group of small cities which were not university 
centers, we find Hibbing, Minnesota, with a death-rate in 1920 of 
8.8; Revere, Massachusetts, with a rate of 7.2; Clifton, New Jersey, 
with a rate of 7.1; and Cleveland Heights, Ohio, with a rate of 6.9. 
These rates are not impossible, but they are so improbable as to 
shift the burden of proof to the office responsible for them. 

Turn for a moment to the conditions in a larger city than Ithaca. 
The death-rate of Buffalo as reported by the city department of 
health, dropped, between 1891 and 1897, as follows: 23.5, 20.0, 19.0, 
16.7, 14.0, 12.7, 12.4: a decrease in six years of 47 per cent. About 
this record the mayor said in one of his annual messages: "The 
death-rate is now lower than that of any other large city in the coun- 
try. The city of Buffalo probably has no prouder distinction than 
the pre-eminent place it occupies among the cities of the country in 
point of public health." In a public address which I gave in Buffalo 
early in 1898 I ventured to question the accuracy of the rate of 12.4 
per 1,000 for 1897. The evidence I had consisted of these items: 
(i) an opinion from our first American expert, J. S. Billings, that the 
chances were at least 50 to i that the death-rate in Buffalo had not 
been below 16 per 1,000; (2) a statement from the statistician of the 
Prudential Life Insurance Company that their experience showed a 
mortality among their policyholders in Buffalo of between i and 2 
per 1,000 above the average in all the cities of New York State; 
(3) the serious omissions in Ithaca and the identity of its law and 
ordinances with those of Buffalo. In my address I claimed not that 
this evidence was conclusive, but only that it shifted the burden of 
proof and warranted a public expression of skepticism. No omissions 
of deaths from the record were demonstrated in Buffalo as they were 



TEE DEVELOPMENT OF AMERICAN VITAL STATISTICS 129 

in Ithaca, and as a result neither position was established to the 
satisfaction of its opponents. The annual report of the New York 
State Board of Health for 1897, which appeared later in 1898, gives 
for each month about the same number of deaths in Buffalo as were 
reported by the city department of health, but in another part reports 
5,013 deaths for the year, an excess of 541. No explanation of the 
discrepancy is to be found in the volume, and letters of inquiry 
recently sent to each office yielded no clue. The large figure can 
hardly be a misprint, because it is consistent with the total for the 
county. The only conjecture I can make is that the larger total 
included belated returns of deaths of non-residents subtracted by 
the city authorities, or deaths occurring outside of Buffalo, but of 
persons who resided in the city. 

At the Census of 1900 the federal authorities obtained returns of 
deaths from enumerators wherever the registered deaths indicated 
a death-rate of less than 15 per 1,000, and checked the results with 
the local registration. In Buffalo the registration records showed 
5,528 deaths, the enumerators 3,135, of which 207 could not be found 
in the registration. Accordingly, these were added. If the result be 
accepted it indicates that in 1900 between 93 and 94 per cent of the 
deaths were registered. After the Census of 1900 it appeared also 
that the estimate of Buffalo's population in 1897 had been seriously 
exaggerated. The result of these combined corrections was to raise 
the Buffalo death-rate in 1897 from 12.4 to 15.5 per 1,000. 

I will not discuss the Chicago death-rate in 1900, over which a 
vigorous argument arose between the city department of health and 
the federal Bureau of the Census. But in 1904 the Weekly Bulletin 
of the Chicago health department, after showing that the average 
age of Chicago decedents in 1872 was 15.2 years, and in 1903 was 
32.1 years, went on to say: "These figures show an increase in the 
average duration of human life in Chicago in thirty-one years of 
in per cent." This is an admirable example of the hoary fallacy 
exploded in Europe in the eighteenth century that the duration of 
life is measured by the average age at death. The average age at 
death depends on two variables, the average age of the living and 
the average duration of life. The average age at death in 1872 was 
low because the population of Chicago then consisted mainly of two 



130 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

classes: young children with a high death-rate, and persons 3 to 55 
years of age with a low death-rate. By 1923 the group of persons 
over 55 years of age with a high death-rate had become numerous 
enough for the deaths among them powerfully to affect the average 
age at death. The figures on which the city health department relied 
furnished no proof that life in Chicago had lengthened. For that 
death-rates by age leading up to a life-table are required. 

In 1915 I had occasion to examine the population statistics and 
vital statistics of the Pacific Coast states and concluded that the 
death records of Washington and the birth records of California were 
vitiated by serious omissions. In subsequent articles the former were 
defended by the health officer of Washington and impeached again 
at greater length. I took the position that a death-rate of 10, which 
was the published rate of Washington in 1910, no more deserves 
serious statistical analysis than does the claim that vaccination does 
not diminish the danger of contracting smallpox. The death-rate of 
Washington was defended mainly on the grounds that the population 
was young and healthy and infant mortality very low. To meet these 
arguments the death-rates in Washington in 1910 for each sex at 
each of eighteen age-periods were applied to a stationary or life- 
table population, yielding a death-rate of 14.2 per 1,000. This meant 
that if the state of Washington had had for a generation a population 
without immigration or emigration and with a constant yearly num- 
ber of births and an equal number of deaths, all other conditions 
remaining identical with those of 1910, the death-rate, instead of 
10.0 per 1,000 would have been 14.2, or 42 per cent higher, the in- 
crease measuring the effect of the favorable sex and age composition 
of the population in 1910 in lowering the death-rate. It meant also 
that under the 1910 conditions, if the death-rate of 10.0 was correct, 
the average length of human life in the state of Washington would 
be 1,000 divided by 14.2, or 70 years, and its median length about 
80 years. In other words, if its true death-rate was 10 per 1,000, 
one-half of all the persons born in the state would live to be more 
than 80 years of age. 

The population of no American state probably is so healthy or so 
peculiarly constituted in the matter of age distribution or both as to 
warrant accepting without careful inquiry a death-rate of less than 



THE DEVELOPMENT OF AMERICAN VITAL STATISTICS 131 

10. In a sparsely settled district it is difficult to secure a record of 
every death, and, in additon to a good law such as all the registration 
states now possess, years of painstaking, intelligent, and tactful ad- 
ministration are required to secure trustworthy results throughout a 
state, including as most states do, extensive sparsely settled districts. 

New York State has in Hamilton County the most sparsely set- 
tled county east of the Mississippi River; for years that county had 
the lowest death-rate recorded in New York State. Its death-rate 
during the first decade of the twentieth century was 10.1 per 1,000 
or three-fifths of the average rate for the state. But during the 
second decade its rate rose to 13.2 per 1,000 while that for the whole 
state fell to 15.6, so that the difference between the state and the 
county was reduced by two-thirds. This change should be ascribed 
to an improvement in the completeness of Hamilton County regis- 
tration, not to an actual increase of sickness and death. 

If my contention that a death-rate of 10 per 1,000, or less, in 
any part of the United States is probably an erroneous rate and 
points to a failure to register all the deaths be approved, it will be of 
interest to ask where such rates occur. Let us confine our attention 
to the sparsely settled rural districts where a death is more likely to 
go unregistered, and ask: What proportion of the area of each death- 
registration state in 1920 was included in rural districts having a rate 
of less than 10? In New England, New York, Delaware, and Mary- 
land every county had a death-rate of more than 10 in its rural dis- 
tricts; one or two sparsely settled counties in New Jersey, Pennsyl- 
vania, and Ohio had a recorded rate of less than 10; those areas in 
Illinois, Michigan, Virginia, South Carolina, North Carolina, Wash- 
ington, and California occupied between one-tenth and one-fourth 
of the state; those areas in Florida, Tennessee, Kentucky, Missouri, 
Colorado, Mississippi, and Louisiana occupied between one-half and 
three-fourths of the state, and in Nebraska nearly four-fifths. 

It is not necessary to accept a death-rate of 10 per 1,000 as the 
limit which divides presumably complete from presumably incom- 
plete registration. No matter how low the limit is set, provided it 
is above zero, a number of counties in the registration area will be 
found to fall below it. Thus Colorado contains three counties with 
a combined population in 1920 of nearly 12,000, and those counties 



132 TEE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

together reported two deaths for the year. They have about two 
persons to a square mile. At the other end of the country is Monroe 
County, Florida, which includes, in addition to the city of Key West, 
800 persons living on 1,100 square miles of mainland and reporting 
two deaths for the year. 

There were 2,011 counties in 1920 within the death-registration 
states, and of these 617, or three-tenths, had a death-rate in the rural 
districts of less than 10 per 1,000. The evidence I have summarized 
leads me to believe that in the great majorityof these 617 counties 
the unregistered deaths were numerous enough to make the pub- 
lished rates incorrect and misleading. What I plead for, however, 
is, not that you accept this opinion, but that if possible you study 
the problem in your own state and form your own conclusion about 
the official rates. 

Let us turn now to the similar question about the completeness 
with which births are registered. Perhaps the best compendious 
test of the accuracy of birth registration comes from a comparison 
between the number of births registered during the year before the 
census day and the number of living children under one year of age 
reported in the census. If both were accurate the number of births 
during the year would exceed the number of children living at its 
close by about the number who had died. If fewer births were reg- 
istered than children enumerated, probably many births were not 
registered. Indeed an excess of about 10 per cent in the births over 
the children living is needed to allay suspicion. Unfortunately this 
test can be used only once in ten years. There were three states, 
South Carolina, Kentucky, and Oregon, in which more children un- 
der one year of age were found living on January i, 1920, than the 
number of births registered in 1919. Two of these three were ad- 
mitted to the birth-registration area in that year. There were six 
others in which the excess of registered births over children living was 
less than 5 per cent, and in which registration was almost certainly 
defective. There were eight in which the registered births exceeded 
the children living by 5 to 10 per cent, and in these the registration 
was open to suspicion. There were six with an excess of births 
amounting to more than 10 per cent. I am disposed to conclude that 



THE DEVELOPMENT OF AMERICAN VITAL STATISTICS 133 

not more than these six states, namely, New Hampshire, Massachu- 
setts, Connecticut, New York, Maryland, District of Columbia, had 
reasonably complete birth-registration. All these states, except 
Maryland, had been in the birth-registration area from the start in 
1915, and Maryland was admitted only a year later. The same 
method of analysis might advantageously be carried into the coun- 
ties, since with births, as with deaths, the sparsely settled districts 
are those in which registration is presumably least satisfactory. In 
nearly every birth-registration state the excess of births registered 
in 1919 over children living January i, 1920, was two or more times 
as great in the large cities as in the rest of the state. Yet if registra- 
tion of births were as complete in the country districts as in the 
cities one would expect the excess in the country to be greater, be- 
cause infant mortality is probably less. 

The subject of the accuracy of registration in sparsely settled 
districts was discussed at the 1923 meeting of the International Sta- 
tistical Institute in Brussels. At the end of the discussion it was 
decided to appoint a committee to examine into the question and re- 
port upon it at the meeting to be held at Rome next October. As I 
am a member of that committee I am especially anxious to get any 
light upon the problem which my colleagues here may be able to 
throw. 

Thus the United States is within sight of a complete system of 
vital statistics developed by a unique method of voluntary co-opera- 
tion between the federal government and the several states and 
cities. I congratulate the Sociological Society, many of whose num- 
bers have aided wisely and effectively in reaching this result. The 
greatest immediate need is for a better popular understanding and 
support of a system which might easily be wrecked, as it has once or 
twice been endangered, by a failure of co-operation. A need hardly 
less pressing is for men and women trained in vital statistics and 
able and eager to interpret the figures thus made available. Is it too 
much to say that we never have had in the employ of the United 
States or of any of the states a person who has rendered to vital 
statistics anything like the service rendered by Farr in England, the 
Bertillons in France, Boeckh in Germany, or Korosi in Hungary? 



134 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

Now the material is becoming available and I felicitate especially the 
rising generation of scholars on the prospects thus opening before 
them. 

Possibly a larger opportunity may grow from the success of this 
modest experiment. Many citizens deplore the present tendency to 
magnify the functions of the federal government at the expense of the 
states. We are in serious danger of carrying that shift to a dangerous 
excess. May not the development to which I have called your atten- 
tion this morning point out a path whereby the government at Wash- 
ington may furnish leadership without authority, and thus slowly 
educate local opinion and guide local action? 

At the present moment, for example, our citizens are hesitating 
between a desire to reduce the evils of child labor and a desire to 
retain state autonomy. Would it be possible to get a uniform law for 
the state regulation of child labor and the state registration of child 
laborers drafted by the groups especially interested in the subject, 
and Congress petitioned to urge the states to adopt the draft? If a 
few states should adopt it and require child laborers to be registered, 
the federal government might build up a registration area for child 
laborers under the leadership of the Bureau of Labor Statistics or 
the Children's Bureau in the Department of Labor, and thus focus 
public opinion and produce the statistics needed to elucidate the 
problem. 

Whether the illustration I have chosen be a happy one or not the 
unique social experiment to which your attention has been invited 
suggests a way for harmonizing state action, in fields where harmony 
is needed, under guidance which no state and no group of private 
citizens, however well organized and familiar with the subject, can 
furnish. 



SECTION ON RURAL SOCIOLOGY 

SIGNIFICANT FACTORS IN RURAL POPULATION 
AFFECTING OUR CIVILIZATION 



FARM POPULATIONS 



JOHN M. GILLETTE, UNIVERSITY OF NORTH DAKOTA 

ABSTRACT 

Farm Populations. The relative decline of rural population is an established fact 
for most advanced nations, including the United States, (i) The assumption that this 
relative decrease in our nation is bad because it proportionally diminishes an inherently 
superior biological stock is unfounded, any such seeming superiority being proved to be 
contingent. (2) The assumption that the effects of this relative decline are bad because 
it proportionally diminishes an inherently superior moral stock is unfounded because 
any seeming moral superiority of rural people is the outcome of a peculiar situation. 
(3) The rapid growth of world-population and the relatively fixed limits of ultimate food 
production bode a crisis sometime in future. A relative decline of rural population might 
conceivably seriously threaten the food supply, however, before that crisis is reached. 
Our nation has already reached the point of marginal agricultural production and needs 
to consider the relation of farming population to food supply. However, the rate of 
natural increase of rural population is approximately double that of urban districts. 
As a result between six million and seven million persons shifted over from rural to 
urban communities during the last census decade, something like 70 per cent being from 
farms. Such a mighty shift must involve serious effects on both city and country be- 
cause, (a) population conditions social life, but not because (6) the shifting population 
is superior or inferior inherently, biologically, or morally. 



This particular subject I am set to discuss is a good example of the presence 
of assumptions and theories in the field of the sciences. The writers on popula- 
tion often disagree in the conclusions they draw from their facts because they 
come to the study with such widely diverging viewpoints. There are those who 
are influenced by the dogma of race superiority, others by that of Nordic super- 
iority, others by that of rural superiority, others by that of the superiority of the 
inherited biological factors over cultural factors. As a result, otherwise excellent 
works are vitiated by unproved assumptions and the conclusions of the various 
authors are divergent and contradictory. 

In this paper I shall assume that the term civilization refers to our present 
society, that the term population connotes group members in their biological 
aspects, and that our civilization is worth promoting and, therefore, discussing. 
I shall confine my attention to the quantitative aspects of farm populations 
because they are more tangible, statistically measurable, and consequently more 
subject to warranted conclusions. But I hasten to confess that my conclusions 
are likely to satisfy no one who expects certitudes. For I find the situation is 



136 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

this. If I throw away all assumptions I can reach no, or few, conclusions. If I 
adopt assumptions, all my conclusions may well be doubted and rejected. What 
is left, then, is to point out that if such and such an assumption is true, the result 
will be so and so; but if another assumption is made, then some other conclusion 
must be drawn. 

We might take as our beginning and basic proposition the sociological con- 
ception of the interdependence of the parts of society. According to this con- 
ception we would expect that, since all parts of our world-society are related to 
each other by reason of their functional interdependence, rural society and 
rural population are in vital and effective touch with all parts of the world and 
are therefore sending their currents and shocks to every part of the world-sys- 
tem. This of course means that nothing in rural society itself or in the conditions 
which determine what that society shall be can be without its influence upon the 
world-system. And since population in its biological aspects and individuals 
making up the population in their psychophysical aspects are such fundamental 
conditioning factors of social acitivities and social life, it is seen that, when viewed 
in general terms, they must be of great moment in affecting the character of 
our world-society. 

There are several features of population viewed quantitatively which deserve 
attention. The first to be considered is the relative decline of farm populations. 
The relative decline of rural population is well known to be a world-phenom- 
enon. During the last century the shifting of the population center of gravity 
from rural to urban districts has proceeded with constantly accelerated rates. 
In 1800 about 97 per cent of the people of Europe were rural, but by 1900 only 
about 25 per cent were classed as rural. In 1800 there were only 14 European 
cities having as many as 100,000 inhabitants, while in 1900 there were 136 such 
cities. 

The population of the United States has undergone a similar shifting be- 
tween 1790, the date of the first census, and 1920. The amount of this shift 
varies according to the criterion of what we regard as city and country popula- 
tions, but the story is always impressive. Using 8,000 as the differential, we 
find 96.7 per cent of the population rural in 1790, and 56.2 per cent in 1920. 
With 2,500 as the criterion, the percentages in 1790 and 1920 were 95.2 and 
51.4. With incorporation as the differential, the percentages are 95 and 40.2. 
Not only is there a relative decline of our national rural population, but an 
absolute decline in sections of the nation. During the last census decade, 25 
per cent of the 48 states, about 70 per cent of the 3,000 counties, and probably 
50 per cent or more of the scores of thousands of townships had fewer rural in- 
habitants in 1920 than in 1910. 

Having established the fact of relative decline of rural populations, let us 
consider the import of a relatively declining rural population for the world. But 
of the many possible effects, let us notice three. 

First, the influences arising from lessening the proportion of a certain kind 
of physical stock. We might make two assumptions here in order to further the 



FARM POPULATIONS 137 

discussion: (a) The rural stock is a superior stock physically and, (b) this 
superiority is due to hereditary causes. We should like to know whether or not 
these assumptions are true. 

Let us cite Professor East as an example of those who believe the rural 
people of the United States are inherently physically superior to those of cities. 
His evidence is based on the army examinations for admission to service during 
the recent war and the mortality and morbidity records. We can substantiate 
his facts. In the army tests, according to the Surgeon General's report, the pro- 
portion of urban recruits rejected was 15 per cent greater than that of rural 
recruits. The average urban death-rate for the ten years, 1910-19, is about 15 
per cent above the average rural rate. Mr. East's explanation of this physical 
superiority is that the rural white inhabitants are dominantly natives of native 
parents, whereas this element in urban populations is in the minority. He 
further concludes that this preponderating native rural stock is almost entirely 
Nordic, since for the first century our immigration was, he says, almost wholly 
British, Scandinavian, and Teutonic. Further, this writer classifies as ignoramus 
or joker those who believe that this physical superiority is due to the conditions 
under which rural people live. 

It appears to me that Mr. East has placed himself in the joker class by 
ascribing the favorable rural mortality differential to the predominance of the 
so-called Nordic stock. He has evidently ignored some important facts. First, 
the tendency manifested among our states for the urban death-rate to become 
and remain lower than the rural rate. Several states have established and main- 
tained an urban death-rate below that of rural districts: Massachusetts since 
1910; New York since 1911 ; New Jersey and Washington since 1915; California 
since 1916. Thus we have five states in which the death-rate is considerably 
lower in city than in country. Neither city nor country have changed their 
proportion of foreign-born materially since this has been true. The predomi- 
nance of so-called Nordics in the country have not prevented the occurrence of 
the higher death-rates in the five states alluded to, nor has the absence of pre- 
dominance of the Nordic element in cities debarred the more favorable mortality 
rate there. 

Second, it seems to be an established fact that urban populations possess a 
greater resisting power to the attacks of pathological germs than do rural popu- 
lations when exposed to the same conditions. Thus when men are drawn to- 
gether in large groups, as in the army, the rural recruits are much tess resistant 
than men from cities. Recent statistics from South America indicate that city 
children have greater resistant power to tuberculosis than rural children. It is 
indicated that the fatality from tuberculosis varies inversely with the prevalence 
of that disease in populations. 

Third, the so-called Nordic stock is a very shadowy and uncertain thing in 
America. To say that rural people are Nordic because they descended from 
British, Scandinavian, and Teutonic stocks is to overlook the fact that the 
majority of those stocks are non-Nordic, if dolichocephaly and brachycephaly 



138 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

are dependable criteria. Mr. East has inferred a biological fact from a cultural 
fact. 

It seems safe to conclude then that any advantage in mortality rates rural 
inhabitants possess is due rather to the conditions under which they live than to 
racial stock. Any loss civilization may sustain from this relative decline arises 
from shifting the center of gravity of population farther toward a higher mor- 
tality rate. But the bow of promise is that cities may more than compensate for 
this by improving their housing and sanitary conditions. 

It may be said that the rural population is made of better moral stuff, so 
that its relative decline decreases the world's supply of this highly desirable 
element. This statement has been made by various writers and deserves ex- 
amination. 

Statistics do show that rates of crime, especially relating to property, and 
of divorce are much lower in rural than in urban districts, and that the marriage 
rates are higher. Seemingly competent studies also indicate that vice, at least 
of the institutional sort, is less rampant in the country. But again we must 
assert that these advantageous characteristics are contingent rather than in- 
herent. To say that there is less crime and divorce in the country because the 
rural inhabitants are inherently superior morally is like saying that because 
there are nine times as many male as of female convicts in our prisons, women, 
by nature, are nine times as good as men. We can admit that boys and girls 
reared in the country develop habits of industry and honesty that may stand the 
strain of urban life at some later date better than those reared under urban con- 
ditions; but we are without proof that the inherent character or moral fiber of 
farming people is superior. 

Any loss to civilization sustained by relatively diminishing this kind of 
population is due to the fact that it is always a misfortune to decrease our sup- 
ply of persistently upright individuals whether they have been made so by 
nature or by the conditions under which they develop. 

The relative decline of rural population may be conceived to be significant 
for the food supply of nation and world. It is difficult to shut our eyes to the 
fact that the growth of populations generally realizes the spirit if not the letter 
of the Malthusian law that populations tend to increase geometrically and sub- 
sistence arithmetically. It is estimated that the population of the world was 
850,000,000 in 1800, and about double that a century later. Another century, 
at the same rate of increase, would yield a world-population of approximately 
3,000,000,000. Professor East makes an elaborate and seemingly fair estimate 
of the possibilities of the ultimate food-producing capacity of the planet, in- 
cluding all land areas and the sea, and arrives at the conclusion that the world 
can produce enough food, by the employment of present known methods of 
production, to supply a population of 5,200,000,000 souls. No one can say when 
this limit will be reached, but we can see that should the rate of increase of the 
past century be sustained, it would be overtaken in about 175 years. 

It may be objected that we of today need have no concern over a possible 



FARM POPULATIONS 139 

food shortage some two centuries hence; that in the past society has always been 
able to make mother earth produce more food when the need arose and that 
doubtless such will be the case in future. Even our social scientists are likely to 
exercise a noble self-restraint and manifest almost as slight emotional excitement 
in the face of this contingency two centuries removed as over the secular ap- 
proach of the moon toward the earth which promises to land that satellite in 
our unwilling lap a few hundred million years hence. 

It may also be objected that the lowering of birth-rates in the more ad- 
vanced nations promises to postpone the arrival of the saturation point of popu- 
lation and that as outlying peoples rise in the scale of social development, they 
too will come under influences making for lower birth-rates. Were birth-rates 
alone concerned in the increase of population, this contention would have more 
weight than it possesses. But death-rates also fall as social development takes 
place and the increase of population does not decline as rapidly as decreasing 
birth-rates alone indicate. The estimates of increase of the world-population in 
future are based on recent years where lowered birth-rates and lowered death- 
rates alike obtain. With the exception of France, the populations of the ad- 
vanced nations increase at a rapid rate, while the chief check to an enormous 
increase among outlying and backward peoples is an exceedingly high death-rate. 

The first ground of optimism, the fecundity of civilization in discovering 
improved methods of producing food, needs scrutiny. According to the best 
methods of agriculture now used, it is estimated that on the average the world 
cannot realize a greater supporting power of production than one person to each 
2.5 acres. The saturation point of 5,200,000,000 inhabitants rests on this figure. 
There is not much likelihood that improved agricultural methods can greatly 
exceed this limit. It may be said that in future industrial chemistry will be able 
to produce sufficient foods without recourse to agriculture. It is unquestionably 
true that many high-grade chemists look forward with great assurance to such 
a time. But it is one thing to be able scientifically to produce foodstuffs from 
inorganic matter and another thing to do so so cheaply that such food can be 
put within reach of all. Gold can now be produced from other metals, but it is 
not commercially advantageous to do so. Further, it may be questionable if 
chemically produced food would be adjusted to the human organism. Osborn 
has shown that unless the animal stomach is supplied with from 1 8 to 20 amino 
acids it is incapable of producing proteids for its own digestive system. So far 
plants have been able to transform the energy of the inorganic into the organic 
far more effectively than man. However, we are merely saying that science has 
far to go before it can supply life-giving food from the inorganic in commercially 
available quantities, not that science cannot do so ultimately. 

This matter of food supply becomes more serious when we consider the 
situation confronting the most populous nations. With the most intensive cul- 
tivation possible, China has reached its limits of production, and were it not for 
its prodigious death-rate which eliminates a half or more of the children, millions 
would starve annually or be forced to migrate. India is in almost the same fix, 



140 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

and Japan has become so populous and is growing so rapidly that the necessity 
for finding a place outside its own territory in which to settle its excess popula- 
tion is making it a disturbing factor in the council of nations. The Western in- 
dustrialized nations of Europe and Great Britain only keep their populations go- 
ing by great importations of food. Only their ability to draw upon the food sup- 
plies of non-industrialized peoples enables them to avert famine and disaster. Our 
own nation seems to be moving toward this same position of dependence. This is 
denoted in the ratios between the average value of foodstuffs imported and 
exported for various sets of years since 1840. These ratios tell a story. About 
1840, we imported twice as much food in value as we exported, one and a 
half times as much by 1860, one and a fourth times as much by 1870, and 
about half as much by 1890. Then we began to return to the prior condition of 
dependence on the outside world for foodstuffs, until by 1920-22 we imported 
almost three-fourths as much as we exported. It might seem that we will con- 
tinue the tendency toward greater proportionate importation of foodstuffs. 

It is rather difficult to determine whether our rate of agricultural production 
keeps pace with the growth of our population. The difficulty is in discovering 
the common denominator for the various food-products. Perhaps the best 
method is to convert the various forms of products into food calories. This was 
done for the average amounts produced for the last three decades and the results 
placed on a per capita basis. The per capita amounts of farm food-products in 
millions of calories were 5.6 for 1890-1900, 6.4 for 1900-1910, and 5.9 for the 
decade 1910-20. We thus find that our food production has somewhat more 
than kept pace with the growth of population. How long we can continue this is 
a question. 

We have much unimproved land in farms to be put under production and 
also considerable areas to be incorporated into farms. By improving all lands in 
farms and by bringing all reclaimable lands into farms and by improving our 
methods of cultivation we should be able comfortably to feed twice or thrice our 
present population. Just when our population will reach the 200,000,000 mark 
is problematical. Pearl's estimate places the date at about 2100. My own cal- 
culations, based on the decrease of our rates of decennial increase, places the 
point at 1970. 

About all we can say today is that sooner or later the world and our nation 
will meet the problem of producing sufficient foodstuffs for their teeming popula- 
tions. The relative decline of farming populations, at least when the point of 
diminishing returns is reached, means a somewhat proportionate reduction of 
food production. Our nation is passing into the condition of being dominantly 
industrial and so is joining the ranks of older nations in being dependent upon 
outside food-supplies. Farmers will not continue to farm if farming is unprofit- 
able. It is the business of states to understand the issue and to see to it that 
farming is sufficiently remunerative to attract and retain the needed farm popu- 
lations on farms. 



FARM POPULATIONS 141 

So far we have noticed some of the possible influences upon our present 
society from the relative decrease of rural populations. Now we shall consider 
another phase of the quantitative aspects of such population. And because our 
time is limited, I shall confine attention to our own rural people. 

The rural population of the United States actually is increasing at a very- 
rapid rate and because of this is able to contribute vast numbers of people to 
cities. This is due to its large rate of natural increase. I have tried to estimate 
the rate of natural increase for the last two decades. The figures for the decade 
1900-1910, for the nation and for the nine divisions, have been published in the 
Journal of Statistics. Of course, I recognize that these rates are only very rough 
approximations due to the meager data existing at that time. 

But recently I have worked out rural and urban rates of natural increase for 
the nation which I believe are much less approximate and fairly near the actual 
situation. The greater degree of accuracy is due to the fact that the registration 
area for some years has covered the major portions of the population. The cen- 
sus data in other particulars also have been greatly improved, so that not only 
can a more accurate rate of natural increase be secured, but a more correct esti- 
mate of the force of the various factors making for increase or decrease of rural 
and urban populations can be made. 

Without going into a discussion of the methods by which the results are 
secured, and the various checking devices applied making for accuracy, the 
results will be given. Perhaps we can best picture the situation by presenting 
the facts relating to the sources of increase of urban population in the United 
States for the last two decades. 

It is to be noted that rural migration, or migration from what the Census 
calls rural districts, represents 45 per cent of urban increase for the last decade, 
and less than 31 per cent for the previous decade; that immigration furnished 
only about 23 per cent of such increase for the last decade, but 41 per cent in the 
previous one; and that natural increase supplied over 23 per cent in the decade 
ending 1920, in comparison with our 20 per cent ten years earlier. 

Put in terms of numbers, rural districts sent nearly 5,500,000 persons to 
cities during the last census decade, and over 3,500,000 in the previous decade. 
We can enlarge these numbers by including what is represented in incorporation, 
nearly a million for each decade. Thus nearly 6,500,000 souls shifted over from 
rural to urban districts in the decade ending 1920. This is a population about 
the size of New York City. It represents more than ten such cities as Pittsburgh 
or San Francisco. That is, the rural districts contribute the population equiva- 
lent of a city a year the size of Pittsburgh or San Francisco. 

What does the rural side of the ledger show as to the source of supply of this 
vast contribution? According to my estimate, the natural increase of rural 
population during the last census decade amounted to 7,100,000, and immigra- 
tion supplied 754,000 inhabitants, a total of approximately 7,850,000. But the 
census credits rural districts with an increase of only 1,600,000. The difference 



142 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

between 7,750,000 and 1,600,000, that is, about 6,150,000, represents the shift 
to cities covered by rural migration and incorporation. Probably 65 to 70 per 
cent of the difference is from farms. 

When we compare the rates of natural increase of city and country, we find 
that the latter is about twice the former. This seems an exaggeration, but my 
estimates make such a difference for both decades, and I have sufficient checks 
on my estimate for the last decade to assure their being conservative. The esti- 
mated urban and rural birth-rates for the last decade were 23.2 and 27.4, the 
death-rates were 15.6 and 13.0, leaving rates of natural increase of 7.6 and 15.2. 

We may say then that the immediate and perhaps greatest influence our 
farming districts are having on modern society consists of this tremendous addi- 
tion of numbers to urban populations. If Professor Hayes is right in holding 
that the form and distribution of population is a technic condition and that tech- 
nic conditions are determinative of social activities, then we must conclude that 
the vast shifts of population indicated exercise a profound effect upon civiliza- 
tion. 

But there are some possible effects of a special nature to be noticed. The 
first is the possible influence on the physical stock of cities. We have previously 
discussed the assumption that because the rural stock, at least the white portion, 
is predominantly native-born of native parents, and because this native stock 
is supposedly Nordic, it is by that fact inherently superior to the urban popula- 
tion physically. Were this assumption true, the urban population of our nation 
would undoubtedly be greatly strengthened by the additions from the country. 
But as we have seen, any physical superiority the rural population possesses is 
likely contingent. It may be that this contingent superiority carries over and 
strengthens the urban population in some directions. On the other hand, as 
was suggested, these rural emigrants may be inferior to urban populations when 
subjected to the toxic infections of intense group life. Second, the rural migrants 
to cities are selected quite largely from the age groups lying between fifteen and 
sixty. In this respect the urban populations are being enriched by the most 
vigorous portions of the rural peoples. This is an undoubted contribution of 
importance and does much to account for the drive and progress of cities. On 
the other hand, our rural population is greatly weakened by the same process. 
Nationally, we may be robbing Peter to pay Paul. 

Third, there is the possibility that rural people are superior to urban people 
in inherent moral fibre. In our previous discussion of this suggestion it was 
regarded as an assumption. We viewed any superiority of morals on the part of 
rural populations as the product of conditions contingent to rural life. Even so, 
it might be that the addition of contingently superior inhabitants to cities would 
be advantageous. No doubt the habits of industry, steadiness, and thrift are 
real contributions. It may be also that devotion to family ties holds over and 
proves contributive. But we have no evidence that those from farms who move 
to cities conduct their business, professions, and political life on higher ethical 
planes than do those native to urban life. And again we have to remember that 



VILLAGE POPULATIONS 143 

rural populations sustain a decided injury in the removal of their most vigorous 
inhabitants. 

Fourth, the migration of vast populations from farms often results in rural 
depopulation. This, in turn, involves increased rural isolation. In this we have 
a devitalization of neighborhood and community life which cannot but be highly 
detrimental. 

VILLAGE POPULATIONS 



C. LUTHER FRY, INSTITUTE OF SOCIAL AND RELIGIOUS RESEARCH, 
NEW YORK CITY 



ABSTRACT 

Village populations. As part of a comprehensive study of American village life, 
the Institute of Social and Religious Research recently made a special inquiry into the 
number and distribution of American villages and also a detailed tabulation of the 1920 
Census data for 177 representative villages. This Census analysis of 177 villages, which 
was the first tabulation of its sort ever made, leads to the following tentative con- 
clusions: (i) The more rapidly the villages of an area are growing, the more nearly 
normal is the age distribution of the population. (2) The friction which so often exists 
between village and open country populations can partly be explained from the fact that 
such a relatively small proportion of villagers are engaged in agricultural occupations. 

(3) There is reason to believe that the differences between village and open country 
populations are so great that in reality the term "rural" as used by the Census had 
better be divided into two distinct subcategories the village and the open country. 

(4) The Census data leads to the hypothesis that certain sections of the country have 
been left with an oversupply of villages by the recent changes in methods of transporta- 
tion which give people in the country easy and quick access to the larger centers. 



The Institute of Social and Religious Research is now engaged upon a com- 
prehensive study of American village life. In addition to actual field work, the 
Institute has undertaken, as a part of this inquiry, a detailed tabulation of the 
1920 Census data for 177 representative villages. It also has undertaken a 
special inquiry into the number, size, and distribution of the villages of the 
United States. Although the field work is still incomplete, it is possible at this 
time to present a few important results of the special village count and of the 
Census analysis of 177 villages. Because the Census analysis was the first tabu- 
lation of its sort ever made, I will indicate to you in a general way not only 
certain conclusions from this study, but also the method of obtaining the data 
from the Census. 

An analysis of village populations is important from many different stand- 
points. It is important merely because of the number of people living in vil- 
lages. It is even more important because village populations are intimately in- 
volved in certain great national problems. The cityward migration of rural pop- 
ulations is one of these problems, since many of the people now moving into our 
great industrial centers are from villages. Town and country relationships cre- 
ate another problem involving village populations. Just as the respective atti- 
tudes of capital and of labor are vital to industrial peace, so the attitude of 



144 



THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 



village to open country and of open country to village are vital to agricultural 
peace. Yet all too often these rural contacts are characterized by friction and 
hostility. 

Probably the most obvious index that comes to mind when considering the 
importance of any population problem is the number of people involved. Un- 



Per G?sjt 
M I DOLE 









f A< 
[PEH 


iC 




* 




* 


1.5 


60 


7O 


5 3 






5 


i 


50 


60 


< 


3 5 


e 


.2 




AO 


50 




7 I 


e 


* 




30- 


.40 




7 1 


6 


i 


' t v 


fi -20- 


". .? 


> J 


7.3 


7.. 


[ 


/' 


1 0- 


**tl 


' 


i" 


7.6 


i 




uNOr 


R IO 




76 . 



| . , PEP 


lOO 


; .2.6 eo- 


TO; 2 3 .' * 


3. 50- 


-co- 35' ; - 


: 5?| . *- 


-so : | 5 . 6 


6.0- ! ! 30- 


-o; . :- 57 .^ ; 


62.' V. 2. > - 


-30- ^ | 9 .7 


.e| ; ^' . o- 


20 f^; : . |,o. 6 


: . . r" 


' . jlO:6 



IO fl 6 -* 



ATLANTIC 



SOUTH 




MIDDLE WEST 





; 


AG 


E . 








55 o 


TO' 2.7 






' 5 


?- 


6* ^43 


t 




6.' 6 


v o- 


-50- 


S 6' 




76 


- 30- 


40- 


lz :3 




73 1 


^5 ,20- 


3 ' /' 


^]5,6 


I 


, 5 | ^ 


f . :, . 


20 f^ 


9,2 


98 


1 


UNOE 


R lO . 


96 



FAR WEST 



FIG. i. Age and sex distribution of the populations in the 177 villages classified 
by regions. 

fortunately, there is no official count of the total number of persons living in 
the villages of the United States. The Census, of course, gives the population of 
every village that is incorporated, but does not list separately the number of 
inhabitants in unincorporated villages. In order, therefore, to ascertain the 
number of villages throughout the country, the Institute counted, state by state, 
all the villages listed in the Rand McNally atlas. Following the definition em- 



VILLAGE POPULATIONS 145 

ployed in all of the Institute's rural studies, the term "village" was limited to 
places with populations ranging between 250 and 2,500. In making the special 
count of the villages listed in the atlas, no effort was made to estimate the size 
of villages whose populations were not actually reported in the atlas because it 
was felt that the village populations thus omitted would tend to neutralize any 
tendency on the part of the atlas to overestimate the size of the villages included. 
This tabulation reveals that the atlas lists 16,434 villages which have an aggre- 
gate population of 11,186,185 persons. 1 This means that slightly more than one 
out of every ten inhabitants in America lives in a village. 

This special tabulation of atlas villages affords an opportunity to determine 
the proportion of American villages that are incorporated. For the United States 
as a whole the number of incorporated villages between 250 and 2,500, as given 
in the Census, is 10,225. If our special count is correct, this means that 62 per 
cent of American villages are incorporated. This ratio, however, shows decided 
regional variations. In the New England states only 7 per cent of the villages 
listed in the atlas are incorporated. This very low ratio is owing to the fact that 
in many parts of New England it is not the custom, as in other states, for vil- 
lages to incorporate as separate municipalities. Figures for the Middle West 3 
show, on the other hand, that 82 per cent of the villages in this region are incor- 
porated, while the South and Far West show 58 and 61 per cent respectively. 

As might have been expected, the larger villages are the ones that are incor- 
porated. This is demonstrated by the fact that although only 62 per cent of 
the villages of the United States are incorporated, nevertheless 8,500,000 people, 
or 76 per cent of America's village population, live in incorporated villages. 

Despite the importance of the village problem, the available information 
about the composition and characteristics of village populations of the United 
States has been quite meager. Although the federal government has been ac- 
customed regularly to collect its census data separately for each incorporated 
village, nevertheless it has never published this information in any detail except 
for places having more than 2,500 inhabitants. For incorporated villages below 
this size it prints only a single figure the total population of each village. To 
say the least, this is not very rich sociological material with wjiich to work. 
As a result, social scientists have been compelled, in analyzing village popula- 
tions, to rely almost entirely upon the findings of individual surveyors. The 

1 These figures confirm to a remarkable extent the estimated village figures pub- 
lished in Morse and B runner's Town and Country Church in the United States, where 
the total number of villages was computed to be 16,981 and their total population as 
11,460,849. 

3 Following the procedure of the Census, the term Middle Atlantic was limited to 
the three states of New York, Pennsylvania, and New Jersey. The South was used to 
include the three Census divisions known as the South Atlantic, the East South Central, 
and the West South Central. The Middle West comprises the two Census divisions, 
East North Central and West North Central, while the Far West covers the Mountain 
and Pacific divisions. 



146 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

difficulty with these individual surveys, however, is not only that the number 
of villages studied has been small, but also that nearly every investigator has 
followed his own particular method of research. 

Because the information about villages has been so meager, the Institute 
of Social and Religious Research decided to undertake, under the direction of 
Dr. Edmund de S. Brunner, a rather elaborate study in this field. From the 
outset it was agreed to limit the inquiry to "agricultural" villages. By an agri- 
cultural village was meant one that was located in a strictly farming area. This 
distinction was made to avoid the complicating factors that would arise if in 
addition to agricultural villages there were included in the same study sub- 
urban villages, cotton-mill villages, mining villages, lumbering villages, etc. 

The original plan of the inquiry provided for the sending of trained investi- 
gators into approximately 150 villages scattered over the country. It was agreed 
that these surveyors should be sent out in teams of two, and that each team 
should spend from two to three weeks in studying each village. It was proposed 
to ask the Institute's field workers not only to write up in detail the particular 
situation and problems they found in every village surveyed, but also to fill 
out a rather elaborate questionnaire calling for precise quantitative facts about 
many aspects of village life. It was hoped in this way to gain insight into the 
distinctive problems of each village and in addition to get data that would be 
strictly comparable for all the villages. It was also proposed in a few villages 
to undertake more detailed and intensive studies. 

This in brief was the village project which the Institute approved in prin- 
ciple in January, 1923. 

But the project had hardly been approved before it was suggested that the 
scope and plan of the study be materially expanded. In a word, the change pro- 
posed was that the data collected by the Institute's field workers be supple- 
mented by an analysis of the 1920 Census figures for the same villages. The 
obvious argument in favor of this plan was that it would supply far more ac- 
curate information about the composition and characteristics of village popula- 
tion than could possibly be obtained by the Institute's field workers. Since the 
field work of the village study is still incomplete, the rest of this paper has nec- 
essarily been limited almost entirely to an analysis of the Institute's special 
tabulation of the 1920 Census data for 177 villages. 

Before the findings of this Census tabulation are presented, a description 
of the method of obtaining the material may be of interest, particularly as this 
tabulation represents the first analysis of its kind ever undertaken by a private 
agency, and because it should, therefore, be of help to other organizations inter- 
ested in special tabulations of Census findings. 

To Dr. Charles J. Galpin, in charge of the Division of Farm Population and 
Rural Life of the Department of Agriculture, belongs the major credit for mak- 
ing possible the Institute's special tabulation. When the idea first occurred to 
me of supplementing the Institute's field study by an analysis of Census data, 
I went to Washington and saw Dr. Galpin. He encouraged me to go into the 



VILLAGE POPULATIONS 147 

matter further, and sent me to see the director of the Census, Hon. William M. 
Steuart. Mr. Steuart was very sympathetic toward the proposed analysis, but 
during the course of my interview with him it developed there was one outstand- 
ing obstacle in the way of carrying out the idea. This difficulty arose through 
the fact that the Census Bureau uses special machines for tabulating census 
data, and that the Institute could not do its own tabulating without incurring 
a prohibitive cost. As soon as Dr. Galpin heard of the difficulty that had arisen, 
he worked out a plan by which, at the request of his division of the Department 
of Agriculture, the Census Bureau did the tabulating work for the Institute. 
This arrangement was satisfactory to the Department of Agriculture because 
it was agreed that the department should receive a complete set of all tabula- 
tions. 

I have mentioned this matter not only because Dr. Brunner and I wish to 
take this opportunity publicly to thank Dr. Galpin for his co-operation in this 
matter, but also because it was felt that such a statement might help to make 
concrete the splendid service that Dr. Galpin, through his Division of Farm 
Population and Rural Life, is rendering research projects in this field. 

The Institute's special tabulation of village data includes a total of 177 
villages scattered widely over the country. Because the Census does not collect 
its data for individual villages that are unincorporated, the study was nec- 
essarily limited to villages which in 1920 were incorporated. This fact, in turn, 
meant that it was impossible to include villages from certain New England 
states, particularly from Massachusetts and Rhode Island, because in those 
states it is not the practice to incorporate villages as separate municipalities. 
All New England villages were for this reason entirely excluded from the study. 
Any person wishing further details about the Institute's special tabulation is 
referred to A Census Analysis of American Villages which has just been published 
as a series of four regional monographs, but which, because of the general in- 
terest in the material, is about to appear in book form. 

Without attempting to summarize the findings of our Census tabulation, I 
should like to point out certain facts revealed by this study which would seem 
to lead to four important conclusions. The first is that the regional variations 
among villages indicate that the composition of their populations is closely cor- 
related with the rapidity of their growth. For purposes of this analysis the vil- 
lages studied were classified, on the basis of their location, into four main groups 
known as the Middle Atlantic, the South, the Middle West, and the Far West. 
The regional variations among these four groups are quite remarkable. For 
instance, the proportion of old people living in the thirty-four Middle Atlantic 
villages is so high that someone aptly characterized these places as old people's 
homes. More than a third of both the men and women living there are forty- 
five years of age and over. In the forty-four southern villages, on the other 
hand, the age distribution of the population is remarkably normal, only 20 
per cent of the men and of the women being over forty-four years of age, which 
is almost identically the proportion found for the United States as a whole. 



148 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

In view of the complex character of village life, it is not reasonable to 
expect that a single explanation will account for all the regional differences 
among villages, nevertheless it is significant that the more rapidly the villages 
of an area are growing the more normal is the age distribution of the population. 
In the Middle Atlantic and the middle western regions, where villages over the 
last twenty years have grown relatively slowly, we find that the age distribution 
of the inhabitants is decidedly distorted. In the South and Far West on the 
other hand, where villages are growing rapidly, we find the distribution of the 
population to be relatively normal. 

The reason why villages in rapidly growing areas have populations whose 
age distributions are relatively normal probably is that in such villages there is 
a real opportunity for young men to build up new enterprises. Therefore it is 
but natural to find a relatively normal number of young people living in these 
villages. In slowly growing villages, on the other hand, economic opportunities 
are relatively scarce. As a consequence, it is but natural that many of these 
village boys and girls, as soon as they reach maturity, go off to the city to make 
their fortune 

The second outstanding fact revealed by our Census analysis is the relatively 
large proportion of villagers who are engaged in manufacturing and trading en- 
terprises and who, therefore, have interests quite different from those of the 
people of the countryside. In each area the Census found the largest group of 
villagers employed in manufacturing. In the South 3 1 per cent of the gainfully 
employed men ten years of age and over were classified under manufacture, 
while in the Middle Atlantic villages this ratio reached 47 per cent. Agriculture, 
on the other hand, only employed from 9 per cent in the Middle Atlantic region 
to 23 per cent in the far western villages. The fact that from 75 to 90 per cent of 
all villagers are employed in other than farming occupations is especially sig- 
nificant because of its bearing upon town and country relationships. Since the 
economic interests of the two populations are different, we have here a partial 
explanation of the friction that often exists between villagers and their neigh- 
bors in the surrounding countryside. 

It may be argued that the differences in occupation between village and 
open-country populations are not so important as we have indicated, since even 
the village industries are based largely upon agricultural products. To a certain 
extent this contention is entirely valid; but it should be borne in mind that the 
farmer is interested in the actual price of farm products, while the village 
manufacturer and trader is interested in a differential price. For example, the 
wheat farmer wants high prices, because if the price of wheat is high his return 
is large, while if it is low his return is correspondingly small. On the other hand, 
the manufacturer of wheat products is interested in buying wheat at the lowest 
possible price. In other words, the village manufacturer of agricultural products 
is interested in paying the farmer the lowest possible price for his products, 
while the farmer is interested in securing the highest possible price. Clearly we 
have here a point of friction between village and open-country populations. 



VILLAGE POPULATIONS 149 

This brings us to the third point which is very closely related to the one 
just discussed. Are the differences between the open-country and the village 
populations so great that in reality the term "rural" as used by the Census had 
better be divided into two distinct subcategories the village and the open 
country? Our evidence, as analyzed thus far, indicates an affirmative answer. 
In addition to the occupational differences just considered, the most important 
Census facts bearing upon this point are probably that of age and sex distribu- 
tions, because so many social and economic conditions are largely determined by 
these. The data show that there are wide variations between the village and the 
open country in both age and sex distributions. For example, the Census reveals 
that in the Middle Atlantic region the ratio of men to women in the farm popu- 
lation is as in is to 100. In our sample villages, on the other hand, there are 
only 88 men to every 100 women. In other words, the proportionate number of 
men is 25 per cent greater in the farm population than among villagers. A 
preliminary study of the marital conditions also shows decided differences be- 
tween the populations of the two areas. While our inquiry has not yet pro- 
ceeded far enough to warrant final conclusions, the material seems to support 
the view that there are fundamental differences between the village and the 
open-country populations. A complete analysis of this whole subject is being 
made by Luther S. Cressman of Columbia University to be submitted as his 
dissertation for a doctor's degree. His study will appear as a part of the final 
volume for the village study. 

The fourth and concluding fact I wish to submit is that the number of vil- 
lages within a given area is so large as to indicate that in certain old established 
sections of the country recent changes in our methods of transportation have 
brought about a condition amounting to a virtual oversupply of villages. Take 
the case in the Middle Atlantic states. The data show that in these three states 
there is a village for every forty- three square miles of territory. In other words, 
the villages in this part of- the country are on the average only six and one-half 
miles apart. 

The very large number of villages in this region can be explained from the 
fact that this area was settled long before the coming of the railroad and the 
automobile. In a civilization where the universal means of communication was 
oxcart or the horse and wagon, it was but natural for agricultural villages to 
spring up at very frequent intervals. With the development of modern methods 
of transportation, however, the whole situation has been changed. A farmer in 
his Ford can now travel many miles where before he could only have gone one. 
As a result, open-country populations are traveling farther in order to trade at 
the larger centers. This is but natural since the larger towns are in a position, 
as Professor Kolb has so clearly shown, to render better and more varied serv- 
ices. As a result, many agricultural villages in the Middle Atlantic region have 
had the economic basis for their existence virtually cut out from under them. 
Here, then, we have an explanation of the fact that the villages in this part of 
the country are growing at only about one-third the rate for the country as a 



ISO THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

whole, In addition, it accounts for the relatively small proportion of young 
people living in these villages. Because the economic opportunities are so lim- 
ited, many village boys and girls in this region leave home and go to the cities 
as soon as they reach maturity. 

If this analysis is correct, it leads to the belief that in the future we can 
expect a decline in the proportion of people living in agricultural villages. In 
New England and the Middle Atlantic states, as well as in certain parts of the 
Middle West where farm populations are either static or declining, this may 
involve not only a relative but an absolute decline in the number of agricultural 
villages. This condition will not be true for the South, since in this region there 
is apparently no oversupply of agricultural villages. The explanation for this is 
probably to be found in the fact that up until the Civil War the South was 
organized on a plantation basis. Even after the War, the old system through 
force of habit was perpetuated for nearly a generation. As a result, there are 
not nearly so many villages in the South as in the older areas farther north. The 
Census shows that there is only one southern village for every 167 square miles, 
compared with a village for every 43 square miles in the Middle Atlantic area. 
Irrespective of the future conditions in any specific area, we believe that because 
of modern transportation methods the rural areas of the future will need rela- 
tively fewer villages than such areas have needed in the past. 



RURAL DEMOGRAPHY 



WARREN S. THOMPSON, SCRIPPS FOUNDATION, MIAMI UNIVERSITY, OXFORD, OHIO 



ABSTRACT 

Rural demography. The lack of certain basic demographic data relating to rural 
communities makes it impossible to study their vital statistics in the thoroughgoing 
way students of rural life would like to do. As a consequence we are compelled to try- 
to get at the facts regarding marriage, death, and birth in a roundabout way which is 
far from satisfactory. The results of our studies are, therefore, often unconvincing and 
sometimes the inferences drawn from the data by different people are quite opposed. 

In spite of these difficulties, however, it seems to the author that several important 
facts can be established beyond reasonable doubt. They are as follows: (i) A larger 
proportion of the rural population than of the urban population marries. Farming is 
still a family occupation. The farm women also marry a little younger than city women, 
thus having a longer period during which they are likely to bear children and they marry 
at a time of life when they are more likely to bear children. (2) The death-rate of the 
rural population is lower than that of the city population. This is particularly marked 
when the rates are refined in such ways that the differences hi age and sex constitution 
of these communities no longer exercise an influence upon them. The city exerts a cer- 
tain positive check on population which the country does not. (3) The birth-rate is 
higher in the rural districts than in the cities. Just as the cities exert a greater positive 
check on population growth they also exert a preventive check which leads to a lower 



RURAL DEMOGRAPHY 151 

birth-rate. The only classes of the city population who have a high birth-rate are the 
newer immigrants who still retain their rural vigor and standards of living. 

The result of these differences in vital rates is naturally a more rapid rate of in- 
crease in the rural population in spite of the almost complete lack of scientific health 
work in the rural districts. 



Statistically, rural communities have been grossly neglected by official agen- 
cies, both federal and state. Consequently it is much more difficult to get any 
adequate view of the vital conditions of rural communities than it is of urban 
communities. This dearth of basic data is my excuse for the lack of precision in 
the treatment of my topic. 

MARRIAGE AND MARRIAGE RATES 

There are no adequate data on marriage and marriage rates in the United 
States. Little can be said, therefore, regarding marriage in rural and urban 
communities. The 1920 Census shows that the proportion of married women in 
the child-bearing ages is somewhat greater in rural communities than in urban 
communities. Of all women 15 to 24 years of age, 34.6 per cent of those in rural 
communities are married, while only 30.4 per cent of those in urban communities 
are married ; for the age group 25-34 the percentages are 82.0 and 72.6 respective- 
ly, and for the age group 35-44 they are 85.8 and 76.0. Unfortunately these 
age groups are so broad that they do not permit even of a rough comparison of 
the effects of female celibacy and delayed marriage upon the fecundity of these 
groups. It must be considerable, however, for Knibbs, in his study of fertility 
and fecundity in Australia, has shown that the average number of children born 
to women decreases more rapidly than the duration of marriage as the age at 
marriage increases. This is also shown by the number of wives who become 
mothers in different age-groups: whereas about 40-45 per cent of all wives aged 
20 to 24 will become mothers annually, the percentage will be about 8 less in 
the age group 25-29, and only about 22-27 per cent in the age group 30-34. 
Thus it is abundantly clear that late marriage, as well as failure to marry, very 
greatly affects the increase of population. Knibbs has also shown that the like- 
lihood of sterility increases rapidly as the age of the woman at marriage in- 
creases. Of women married at 17.11 years of age and who have been married 
25 years, thus having generally passed the child-bearing period, only 2.5 per cent 
will be sterile; of those married at 22.9 years of age and who have been married 
20 years, 5.0 per cent will be sterile; of those married at 27.6 years of age and 
who have been married for 15 years, 10.0 per cent will be sterile; of those married 
at 31.6 years of age for ten years, 20 per cent will be sterile; and of those married 
at 37.1 years of age for 5 years, 50 per cent will be sterile. 

So far as I am aware, we have no comprehensive data in this country on the 
age at marriage of urban and rural women nor upon the duration of their mar- 
riages, but the percentages given above show that more rural women marry, 
and the probability is that they marry somewhat earlier. A New York report 



152 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

recently issued gives the percentages of brides marrying at given ages for urban 
and rural areas (outside of New York City) in Table I. 

TABLE I 

PERCENTAGES OF BRIDES IN NEW YORK (OUTSIDE OF NEW 

YORK CITY) MARRYING AT GIVEN AGES IN 

URBAN* AND RURAL COMMUNITIES 



Age at Marriage 


Urban 


Rural 


ic in 


21. < 


27. 3 


2O 24. 


3O O 


37 O 


2<J 2O 


IQ 8 


17 3 


3034 


8.3 


6 6 


3 <? 3Q . 


4 8 


4r 


4O44. . . . . 


2 8 


2 <J 









* Urban communities are certain cities listed in the Report, rural 
communities are all other places; the dividing line is at about 10,000 
population. 

These data indicate that the proportion of all brides marrying under 25, and 
particularly under 20, is somewhat greater in the rural districts. If this should 
be true for the rural population of the country as a whole it would help to ac- 
count for its more rapid rate of increase which I shall discuss later. 

DEATH-RATES 

A discussion of rural death-rates must be prefaced by the statement that all 
cities and villages having less than 10,000 inhabitants (8,000 in Life Tables for 
1 901) are classed as part of the rural population in the recent mortality reports 
of the census. In my opinion, however, the error in the rural death-rate thus 
introduced is less than that due to the differences in age and sex constitution in 
different types of communities. That this latter error is considerable is brought 
out very clearly in the United States Life Tables prepared by Professor Glover. 
In Table II, I have abstracted a few of the significant facts for urban and rural 
communities in 1901 and 1910, and also for two large cities. These data show 
how many males and females would be left alive at given ages out of 100,000 born 
alive at a given time. 

At every age given, the rural communities have a larger proportion living 
than the cities, and Boston and New York City have a still smaller proportion 
living than cities as a whole. Unfortunately similar data based on the 1920 
Census are not yet available. The best we can do with the Life Tables thus far 
issued is to group together certain states which are distinctly alike and compare 
these groups. For this comparison I have chosen the four following groups: 
Group I, the aggregate for fourteen large cities; Group II, Connecticut, Massa- 
chusetts, New York, New Jersey, and Pennsylvania, representing states highly 
industrialized and urbanized; Group III, Illinois, Indiana, Ohio, Michigan, and 



RURAL DEMOGRAPHY 



153 



Wisconsin, representing states about midway between Group II and the follow- 
ing group (IV), consisting of Kansas, Kentucky, Minnesota, North Carolina, 
and Tennessee, representing states distinctly rural, although they, of course, 
have considerable city population. 

Table III shows the number of survivors at the different ages out of 100,000 
born alive at a given time. 

TABLE II 

NUMBER OF WHITE MALES AND FEMALES LIVING AT A GIVEN AGE IN URBAN* AND 

RURAL COMMUNITIES IN THE REGISTRATION AREA, 1901 AND 1910; ALSO FOR 

ALL CLASSES OF THE POPULATION IN NEW YORK AND BOSTON 



YEAR OF AGE 


WHITE MALES 


WHITE FEMALES 


BOSTON (ALL 
CLASSES) 


NEW YORK CITY 
(ALL CLASSES) 


Urban 


Rural 


Urban 


Rural 


Males 


Females 


Males 


Females 


o-i 

JOOI 


100,000 

100,000 

76,124 
79,783 

73,420 
77,469 

68,112 

73,277 

61,142 
66,970 

52,320 
57,976 

40,299 
45,080 

34,484 
27,154 


100,000 

100,000 

83,803 
85,288 

81,306 
83,031 

76,896 
78,915 

72,303 
74,305 

66,589 
68,435 

58,003 
59,375 

42,648 
42,994 


100,000 
100,000 

79,122 
82,390 

76,601 
80,268 

71,745 
76,438 

65,547 
71,075 

58,045 
63,936 

47,023 
52,658 

31,128 
34,769 


100,000 
100,000 

85,868 
87,398 

83,254 
85,321 

78,228 
81,191 

73,012 
76,53i 

67,250 
70,964 

58,874 
62,316 

44,688 
46,845 


100,000 

100,000 

73,926 
79,464 

71,000 
77,097 

64,967 
72,846 

57,665 
65,673 

48,817 
55,683 

36,752 
4i,9i3 

21,211 
24,207 


IOO,OOO 
100,000 

76,799 
82,367 

73,915 
79,8l6 

68,659 
75,694 

62,033 
70,109 

53,996 
62,017 

42,542 
49,879 

26,903 
31,846 


100,000 

100,000 

73,618 
78,652 

71,032 
76,389 

65,311 
72,114 

56,947 
64,868 

46,617 

54,342 

33,784 
40,275 

18,973 
22,814 


IOO,OOO 
100,000 

76,620 
80,932 

74,223 
78,875 

69,209 
75,o63 

62,288 
69,278 

53,890 
61,372 

4i,733 
49,io5 

25,593 
30,739 


IQIO 


9-10 

IQOI 


1910 . 


19-20 

1901 


1910 . 


29-30 

IQOI 


IQIO 


39-40 

IQOI 


IQIO 


49-50 
IQOI 


IQIO 


59-60 

1901 

1910 


69-70 

IQOI . 


IQIO . . . 





* Urban includes all places of 8,000 or more in 1901 and of 10,000 or more in 1910; all other places 
are rural. 

The cities have the lowest survival rate, the highly urbanized and indus- 
trialized states come next. These are followed by the semi-industrialized states, 
and the rural states have considerably the highest rate. 1 wo other ways, both of 
them roundabout and unsatisfactory, of showing that preponderantly rural 
states have a lower death-rate than those with larger urban populations are 
shown in Table IV. 

In the last column of this table the relative adjusted rates of these different 
groups are shown, using the rate for the big cities as 100. We see here that the 
rates for all the other areas are considerably below that for cities, about 10 per 



154 



THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 



cent in the case of the highly industrialized states, about 18 per cent in the case 
of the group which is next in industrialization and urbanization, and about 25 per 
cent in the distinctly rural states. Certainly these differences must be due large- 
ly to the degree of industrialization, although there are, of course, other factors 
entering which need consideration before one can say exactly what proportion 
of this difference is due to the fundamental differences between country and 

TABLE III 

NUMBER OF PERSONS (MALES AND FEMALES) LIVING AT GIVEN AGES, OF 100,000 
BORN LIVING, FOR CERTAIN CITIES AND THREE SELECTED GROUPS OF STATES. 
LIFE TABLES FOR UNITED STATES, 1920. ALSO INDEX NUMBERS SHOWING PRO- 
PORTIONS OF SAME. WHITE POPULATION ONLY 



Group No. 


Males 


Females 


Males 


Females 


Males 


Females 


Males 


Females 






E 


i 


2 


2 


2 


3 


2 


I (14 cities) . . . 


oo 4.76 


O2 4l6 


84,. 100 


86.760 


81 103 


83,814 


78 382 


78 ?77 


II 


QO. <84 


02. <OO 


8?. 147 


87, SQ2 


82. 243 


84,878 


77. ^17 


7Q 8o2 


Ill 


oi .643 


03, 380 


86,716 


88,912 


83,660 


8^,014 


7O.3O8 


8o,6ic 


IV 


Q7 l6l 


04. 33O 


88,720 


oo 326 


8c 7O4 


87 286 


81 146 


81 627 


I (14 cities) 


1,000 


I ,OOO 


1,000 


1 ,000 


I,OOO 


I,OOO 


1,000 


i ,000 


II 


I OOI 


I OOI 


I OI2 


I OIO 


I OI4 


I .OI4 


I O2O 


I OI7 


HI 


I .OI3 


I ,OII 


I O3I 


i .02^ 


1 .032 


I O2 " 


I O4O 


I O27 


IV 


1 .020 


I ,O2I 


i .o^t; 


i .042 


1 .0^7 


1 .042 


1 .063 


I .O4O 






















4 


2 


5 


2 


6 


2 


7 


2 


I (14 cities) 
II 


70,031 

71 , 57Q 


73,042 
74,417 


60,959 
63,665 


65,351 

67,402 


46,395 

^O, 727 


52,557 
cc,386 


26,260 

31 .2^ 


32,4H 

3";,6i8 


Ill 


74,146 


75,266 


67,642 


68,932 


56 , 702 


^8,643 


31,71"? 


40 , \oo 


IV 


7 "s . QQQ 


7<.67Q 


60 Q<?3 


60.463 


CQ. 7QI 


<O 743 


41 . 486 


42 <I2 


I (14 cities) 
II 


I,OOO 
I ,O2 1 


I,OOO 
1 .010 


1,000 

i .04 s ; 


1,000 
1 ,032 


1,000 
I ,O04 


I,OOO 
1 ,O<?4 


I,OOO 

1,186 


1,000 
I , IOO 


Ill 


i,o<>8 


1 ,030 


I , IIO 


I,O<< 


I, 224 


1, 117 


I, 204 


I . 2"?4 


IV 


1,084 


1,037 


1,149 


1,064 


1,289 


1,137 


I,'?74 


1 . 312 





















city. We need detailed life-tables for strictly rural and urban communities be- 
fore we can get at the fundamental differences between them in the matter of 
healthfulness. 

In 1915 Professor Willcox prepared a special report on vital statistics for 
the New York State Department of Health. In this report he prepared standard- 
ized rates for New York City and the rest of the state. He found that for every 
year for which this rate was calculated (1898-1915), New York City had a con- 
siderably higher standardized rate than the rest of the state, although for six 
years, beginning in 1910, the crude rate in New York City had been lower than 
in the rest of the state. To find out just what the strictly rural death-rate was, 



RURAL DEMOGRAPHY 



155 



a study was made by the Department of Rural Social Organization at Cornell 
in conjunction with the state department of health of the data on deaths in the 
rural part of one county. This study was made in great detail, and the area 
selected was believed to be typical of much of agricultural New York. This 
study showed that, whereas the crude rate for the rural part of this county for 
the ten years ending in 1920 was 15.13, the standardized rate was 10.66. The 
standardized rate for New York State outside of New York City, worked out by 
Professor Willcox for 1915, was 13.6. Since this was the lowest rate in any year 
for the five years beginning in 19*1 1, it seems unlikely that the average for the ten 
years ending 1920 would be much below the rate for 1915. We see then that the 
standardized rate for this rural area was only a little over two-thirds that of the 
crude rate, and about 3.0 below that for the state (exclusive of New York City). 
This study also contains data on deaths from specific diseases, which show in 

TABLE IV 

DEATHS PER 100,000 FROM CERTAIN DISEASES IN SAME GROUPS AS GIVEN 

ABOVE; ALSO ADJUSTED DEATH-RATES AND INDEX BASED ON 

ADJUSTED RATES (WHITE POPULATION) 



Group No. 


Deaths per 100,000 of 
Population, from 9 
Diseases Constituting 
63.3 per cent of All 
Causes of Death 
(1920) 


Adjusted Death-rates 
per i ,000 of Popu- 
lation(iQ2o; 


Index 


I (12 cities)* 


Q3O 


M7 


IOO O 


II 


878 


13. 1 


OO 2 


Ill 


74.6 


12 I 


82 2 


IV 


711 


II I 


7 e < 











* Only twelve of the fourteen cities given in the preceding table are included here. 

what respects this rural community differs from the state as a whole and from 
the cities. Such data are indispensable in formulating a health program for any 
rural district. 

Enough has been said here to show that although such death-rates as we 
now have do not give us a true picture of the mortality situation in rural com- 
munities and do not enable us to make the comparisons between different types 
of communities which are needed, yet they do clearly indicate that rural death- 
rates are considerably below urban death-rates. Furthermore, it is well to point 
out that such advantages as rural communities possess in this respect are not 
due, except in rare instances, to any organized health-work. The advantage in 
this respect is all in favor of the cities, so that more favorable mortality rates in 
the country must reflect conditions naturally more favorable to human living. 
In other words, even such meager information on this point as we now possess 
points to the city as bringing into operation some positive checks to population 
growth not present in the rural community. 



156 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

BIRTH-RATES 

When we turn to the consideration of birth-rates, we find even more meager 
information than in the case of death-rates. For one thing* birth registration 
is newer than death registration and is harder to enforce, especially in the rural 
districts. Furthermore, for only one year (1920) has the Bureau of the Census 
given us any data enabling us to compare the size of family in rural and urban 
communities. As these data now stand, they are of comparatively little value, 
because they show us only the average number of children born to mothers 
according to the occupation of the father. However, I shall give these averages 
for several large groups for what they are worth. The group of workers having 
the highest average number of children ever born to mothers in the year 1920 
was mine operatives with 4.3 ; next come farmers with 3.8; laborers in manufac- 
turing and mechanical industries follow with 3.7; the semi-skilled workers in the 
same industries average 3.0, while the managers, superintendents, etc., of these 
workers average only 2.5. Bankers, brokers, money-lenders, and professional 
men as a class average about 2.3. Thus we see that the two groups which are 
distinctly rural farmers and miners have the highest average, and those 
which are most completely urbanized the managerial and professional groups 
have about the lowest average. 

The crude birth-rates given for the white population in the latest report on 
births are 22.1 for cities and 22.4 for rural districts (including all places of 
10,000 or under). In my opinion, for purposes of comparison these rates are 
largely useless because there are several ways in which they are not representa- 
tive of the groups we are trying to compare. This is manifestly the case when 
we find that in 1920 there were 5,378,644 children under five in a rural white 
population of 44,200,831, and only 4,995,277 children of the same age in an 
urban white population of 50,620,084. The percentages which these children 
form of their respective populations are 12.2 and 9,9. 

In view of our inability to tell much about rural birth-rates from the pub- 
lished reports I will attempt to bring together in brief form certain other data 
which I believe will throw some little light on the relative rates in the city and 
the country, without attempting to express the results in definite rates. 

Baber and Ross found in their study of the size of families from which 
college students come, and collateral lines of the same families (chiefly Wisconsin 
students), that farmers had an average of 4.24 children; business men, 2.91; 
clerical workers, 2.61; and professional men, 3.19, when only fertile completed 
families are included. Unskilled laborers had 3.75, but the data for them are 
too meager to be of much value. Thus we see that farmers had much larger 
families on the average than the other classes adequately represented by stu- 
dents attending college. 

A study upon which we are engaged at the present time agrees with these 
findings in general, but being somewhat more extensive shows that there are 
marked differences between farmers in different parts of the country. We found 



RURAL DEMOGRAPHY 157 

that in the northern states the average for professional men was about 3.3; for 
those engaged in agriculture it varied from 3.6 in New England to 5.3 in the 
west North-Central states; for those engaged in trade and in managerial posi- 
tions (probably almost identical with business above), it averaged about 3.3 to 
3.4. In the southern states the figures for whites are: farmers or those engaged 
in agricultural occupations, about 6.2; professional workers, almost 5; trade and 
managerial positions, about 4.4. 

It should also be borne in mind in considering these averages and in trying 
to see what they mean in terms of the natural increase of the different classes 
that a larger percentage of rural women than of city women marry, as was 
pointed out above, and that fewer of them are sterile after marriage. Baber and 
Ross found about 10 per cent of the farmers' families childless, while Von 
Tungeln, in his study of an Iowa county, found only 8.1 per cent of farm owners* 
families childless, while 24.1 per cent of their wives were still under 35. It would 
seem a safe assumption that not over 5 or 6 per cent of these families will 
ultimately be childless. Several studies giving us data on this point show that 
the average percentage of childless families for the clean-handed occupations 
varies from 13 to 22, probably averaging more than twice as high as for farmers. 
In the study of the Immigration Commission about i\ to 3 \ times as many white 
women of native-born parentage were sterile in the cities as in the rural counties. 
This difference between the cities and the rural districts is not so great for the 
foreign-born nor for the children of the foreign-born. Taking the country as a 
whole, therefore, it seems quite conservative to say that the proportion of sterile 
married women is twice as great in the cities as in the rural districts. 

Still other evidence supporting the position here taken is to be found in the 
proportion of children to women in the cities as compared with the rural dis- 
tricts. There is no state in the union in which the proportion of children to 
women is not higher in the rural districts than in the urban districts, with the 
exception of Rhode Island, where the rural population is so small as to be 
negligible. In the white population of the United States as a whole in 1920 there 
were 391 children under five to 1,000 women aged fifteen to forty-four in the ur- 
ban communities, and 580 in the rural communities. If we take only married, 
widowed, or divorced women of the same age the proportions are 629 and 873, 
respectively. The excess in the rural districts being 48.3 per cent in the case of 
all women, and 38.8 per cent in the case of married, widowed, or divorced women 
only. The second proportion is more significant if we are considering size of 
family, the first if we are trying to get some idea of the relative rates of growth 
of the two classes. 

The differences between rural and urban communities may be shown still 
more strikingly by comparing the large cities those having over 100,000 pop- 
ulation in 1920 with the rural population of certain states and with rural 
counties in other states. 

As was said above it is not possible to get as detailed information for the 



THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 



rural districts as for cities, hence certain omissions in Table V. It shows con. 
clusively, however, that on whatever basis the comparison is made the people 
who live under more open conditions have more children than the city dwellers. 

TABLE V 

THE PROPORTION OF CHILDREN o~6 TO 1,000 WOMEN 18-44; ALSO OF CHILDREN 

0-4 TO 1,000 WOMEN 15-44, AND TO 1,000 MARRIED, WIDOWED, 

AND DIVORCED WOMEN 15-44 (1920)* 



Class and Locality 


Proportion of 
Children 0-4 to 
i, ocxj Married. 
Widowed, and 
Divorced Wo- 
men 15-44 


Proportion of 
Children 0-4 to 
1,000 Women 
15-44 


Proportion of 
Children 0-6 to 
1,000 Women 
18-44 


United States (white only) 
Urban 


620 


7,01 




Rural 


877 


180 




All cities of 100 ooo or more 


581 


363 


113 


White 




770 




Negro . . 




1 80 




Farm population of 8 selected counties 


GOO* 


104 




Maine 
Urban . . 


64.2 


178 




Rural 


74.6 


101 




Pennsylvania 
Urban . . . 


601 


4.17 




Rural 


04? 


621 




9 rural counties 






016 


25 counties not including places of 10,000 
or more 






04,7 


20 counties having no city of 10,000 or 
more 






960 


Ohio 
Urban 


r 7 6 


4O3 




Rural ... . ... 


781 


127 




15 rural counties 






844 


34 counties not including places of 10,000 
or more 






8<i 


39 counties having no place of 10,000 or 
more 






740 


West Virginia 
Urban 


607 


4OO 




Rural 


068 


7l6 




6 mining counties not including places of 
10,000 or more 






1108 


12 rural counties . .... 






1208 


Kentucky 
Urban 


/no 


^8 




Rural 


870 


610 




7 rural counties 






ITT 1 


7 mining counties 






IX 33 


Kansas 
Urban 


116 


166 




Rural 


700 


no 













* All classes of the population are included here unless otherwise specified. 



RURAL DEMOGRAPHY 159 

I will not attempt to discuss the reasons for this in any detail at this point, but 
will mention what I believe to be the most important factors in the situation. 

1. Children are of greater economic usefulness on farms than they are in 
towns and cities, thus being less of an economic burden. 

2. Country girls are better trained as home-makers than city girls and have 
less opportunity to enter other kinds of work, consequently they marry earlier 
with the expectation of raising a family. 

3. "Keeping up with the Joneses" is not as likely to be regarded as essential 
by country women as city women. It is too easy to find out a farmer's real 
economic status for his wife to be able to pull the wool over the eyes of her 
neighbors. 

4. Farming requires a settled life, and children do not constitute the sole 
tie of the farm woman to her home as they often do in the case of the city woman. 
Most farmers have to be at home 365 days in the year whether there are children 
or not. 

5. Farm people are constantly working with the natural generative processes 
of nature. They regard reproduction in plants and animals as natural, and it 
would be strange if they took any other attitude towards human reproduction. 

6. It is possible that there has been a selective influence in cityward migra- 
tion, leading the less philoprogenitively inclined to seek the city. One wonders, 
in this connection, whether love of comfort and safety is a "linked' ' trait with a 
weak philoprogenitive tendency. 

7. It seems probable that country people are less affected than city people 
by the numerous causes which lead to involuntary sterility. 

NEGROES 

Before summing up what has been said I wish to compare very briefly 
negroes with whites in respect to their vital statistics. Negroes show about the 
same marital conditions as the white population, except that the women marry 
somewhat younger. In regard to their death-rate, however, they vary markedly 
from the white population. On the basis of every comparison possible the negro 
has a death-rate considerably in excess of the white population. This excess, 
which in crude rates in the cities generally runs from 50 to 70 per cent, fails to 
about 30 to 40 per cent in the rural districts. It shows up in the complete ex- 
pectation of life at time of birth in an astonishing manner. Whereas this is 55.53 
years for all white males in the registration area in 1920, it is only 40.46 years 
for negroes living in states having less than 4 per cent negroes and 46.39 years 
in states having over 5 per cent negroes. In general this contrast in the two 
classes of states is between negroes in the cities of the North and the rural dis- 
tricts of the South. The life-expectation of negro males is still less in large cities, 
being only 38.45 years. For females the differences are about the same, although 
the expectation of life is greater for females in both cases. 

The birth-rate of negroes generally exceeds that of the whites in the rural 
districts and often in the cities, but when one comes to measuring the increase 



160 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

of the two groups by the proportion of children to women one finds a different 
situation. In cities of over 100,000 population the proportion of children under 
five to i ,000 women 15-44 is only 189, or only one-half that of the whites; in 
smaller cities it is 326, and in the rural districts it is 555. This proportion is 
probably too low in all cases, but particularly in the rural districts, due to errors 
in reporting age, but when the 555 of the rural negroes, most of whom live in 
the South, is compared with the 632 of the rural whites in the same area it is 
quite clear that the latter are increasing more rapidly. This conclusion is, of 
course, in complete agreement with what the regular census enumerations have 
shown for several decades, viz., that the negro population was not increasing 
as rapidly as the white population with which it lived, by excess of births over 
deaths, and that as a consequence the negro population was becoming steadily 
a smaller proportion of the whole. 

CONCLUSION 

If the data regarding deaths and births given above are reasonably accurate, 
the natural increase of population is considerably greater in the country than in 
the city. The birth statistics for the registration area show this to be the case 
to a certain extent, but I do not believe they do so adequately for reasons al- 
ready indicated. For the first time a vital index for certain groups of women has 
been calculated in the 1922 report on birth statistics. In this, however, the con- 
trast is between native and foreign-born women by states, so that it throws but 
little light on the question we are discussing. Furthermore, as a vital index of a 
whole population living in a given area it seems to me to be open to several ob- 
jections, the most serious of which is that it is based upon only a small part of 
the population living there. If this fact is borne in mind it will be interesting to 
note that six states which are largely agricultural have an average vital index 
of 2723.0 for native white women 15-44, and 2418.4 for foreign-born white 
women; while six states which are quite highly industrialized and urbanized 
have 1739.1 and 2501.7 for these two groups, respectively. The average rates 
for the entire United States are 2040.9 for native women and 2395.3 for foreign 
women. Thus we see that the rural native women have the highest rates. It 
would seem clear beyond contradiction that from the standpoint of population 
growth, the rural communities stand at the top of all groups in the United States, 
and the incontrovertible conclusion is that rural conditions more nearly meet 
the vital needs of human life than urban conditions. If this is true now, how 
much more significant it is that sanitary and medical science have scarcely 
begun to minister to rural needs. It seems probable to me that the next genera- 
tion will see even greater differences in the vital conditions of rural life, and 
because the new immigrants who are contributing most, if not all, of the city 
increase are being shut out. 

The deadliest enemies of man at the present time are not disease, war, and 
famine, but the -industrial conditions of the cities. They not only take their 
heavy toll in deaths, but prevent their victims from participating in the future 
because they sterilize them. 



THE SHIFTING BASES OF RURAL SOCIOLOGY 161 

THE SHIFTING BASES OF RURAL SOCIOLOGY 



WILLIAM L. BAILEY, NORTHWESTERN UNIVERSITY 



ABSTRACT 

The shifting bases of rural sociology. The trend of our civilization has been con- 
ditioned by two great population movements the rural, the urban. These two fields 
traditionally contrasted have under modern conditions and especially in this country 
been vitally related through population movements. Rural migration to cities and the 
suburbanization of the cities are now active. The rural sociologist must now emphasize 
the relation of city and country, and pass from the urban to the suburban concept. 
Rural sociology is still obsessed of the open country and the agricultural village, but 
recently has given attention to town and country relations. The new and only profitable 
agriculture that is assuming importance in the regions about large cities provides the 
basal conditions for attractive rural life. Attention is turning to progressive and growing 
rural districts, and away from the traditional emphasis on decadence and depopulation. 
Current studies in rural sociology indicate recognition of gradations within the field and 
of the complex and growingly vital relationships of rural and urban. 



The general topic of the Conference, "The Trend of our Civilization," is 
particularly timely. Current fundamental social changes are no longer being 
regarded as "reconstruction" problems, but rather as evidences of basal and 
persistent tendencies of Western civilization. The limitation of treatment large- 
ly to American conditions is less limiting than might appear. For this country 
is a veritable sociological laboratory for modern life. Probably no other coun- 
try even as the result of war conditions is experiencing fundamental social 
change in the degree that this country has been in the last decade. 

This is alike true of urban and rural life. The papers under discussion are 
suggestive of the fact that our civilization has been conditioned by two great 
population movements, not only as a whole but within each national unit 
the pioneering of new rural areas, and the growth of cities in its wake. American 
life is the outstanding instance. The papers have not treated trend so much as 
conditions but throughout the categories of urban and rural have been em- 
ployed, and many evidences of the close and vital relationship of these have been 
brought out. 

Perhaps this latter is their chief contribution. 

Their approach, emphasis, and tendency in comparison has been rural. 
This is especially justifiable under American conditions. For our urban popu- 
lations are still largely externally recruited, and mainly from the still preponder- 
ant rural element in the national life. We are still only in process of transition 
from the rural to the urban stage of national evolution. Background and fore- 
ground are in balance. 

There should then be no trace of prejudice in favor of the rural arising from 
a possibly less complete understanding and appreciation of the urban phase of 
American life. For the relationship between rural and urban has been altogether 
unique in this country from the first; they have developed together. The very 
magnitude of the population movements still connecting the two is evidence 



162 TEE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

that they must be thought of together. This relationship is increasingly inti- 
mate and far-reaching as we are passing over into what may be called a "sub- 
urban" stage in which the centralizing urban movement begins through various 
forms of decentralization to urbanize the rural as for long the rural did the 
urban. So that under American conditions it is imperative that the rural so- 
ciologist be almost as conversant with city conditions as of those in his own 
special field. The recognition of these facts will safeguard judgments on the 
trends detected in the analysis of the rural field. 

Such safeguarding is especially necessary when the data are relatively lim- 
ited and the bases of their collection so various. Considerable is left necessarily to 
assumption and probable conclusion, and comparisons are likely to be invidious. 

In few, if any, countries of the Western world are limitations of fundamental 
social data so great as in our own. Basal demographic or vital statistics are 
naturally difficult to secure where population movements have been so great 
and where social agencies, such as the church and the school, leave to political 
governmental agencies the entire and well-nigh impossible task. 

Students of rural demography are indeed handicapped as compared with 
those of the larger cities. But this relative disparity of information may have 
its compensations for the ruralite. For combined with the fact that the country 
phase of the rural has been musch better known through surveys than the 
village phase, and that the larger industrial cities represent urban conditions in 
their extreme form, the resulting judgments are liable to prove unduly favorable 
to the rural. 

Too great stress cannot be laid on the immediate necessity of representative 
local studies, and on the establishment in connection therewith of systems of 
local record on the part of other than governmental agencies. A flood of light 
on the neglected village phase of the rural has resulted from the recent studies 
of the Institute of Social and Religious Research, made possible by the co- 
operation of Dr. Galpin's office in the Department of Agriculture in securing 
available but hitherto unutilized materials from the census. The recent publica- 
tion of corresponding data for eight selected counties, isolating farming popula- 
tion and displaying their social characteristics, is another case in point. But 
rural social agencies could contribute local records, such as many countries 
abroad afford, that would place rural sociology upon a sound scientific footing. 

This is significant in view of the fact that the control and direction of vital 
and demographic conditions in all probability ultimately depend on cultural 
factors which are not largely within the sphere of government as such, nor even 
in the realm of health alone. 

The cardinal question may also be raised as to the continued legitimacy of 
the terms and categories "urban" and "rural." As these spheres of life inter- 
penetrate, the significance for social interpretation of these terms changes. They 
cannot certainly be employed as traditionally they have been, but may only be 
used for purposes of distinction, not separation. Comparisons of spheres of life 
so related by population movements are scarcely valid. 

The bases of rural sociology have been rapidly shifting. Less than a decade 



THE SHIFTING BASES OF RURAL SOCIOLOGY 163 

ago the rurban concept was developed, and still largely persists. A town and 
country terminology became common, recognizing more and more fully the 
interrelationship of countryside and center. Open-country life of the traditional 
American type begotten of pioneer expansion was recognized to be passing, and 
the various grades of centers, especially those of rural size and type, were real- 
ized to have a growing significance for rural life. Growing knowledge of rural 
civilizations in other parts of the world and in other historical periods combined 
with intensive studies of American localities conduced to this. It would seem, 
however, that at the present time there is need for attention being directed upon 
a new phase, what might be called the "suburban rural," noting especially the 
relationships of the entire rural field, including open-country hamlet, village, and 
town, to the larger centers of city grade and type. 

For American life is undoubtedly moving quickly into a third stage the 
suburban superseding, in a sense, the rural and the later urban stages. And 
rurban rural life will more and more be recognized to be suburbanized, as the 
urban, and especially the larger cities, render the rural districts tributary eco- 
nomically, socially, and culturally. This influence has already transformed very 
considerable regions in all parts of the country. It cannot but be otherwise as 
the urban preponderates in the national balance. 

The scope of what may broadly be called the suburban sphere is by no 
means comprehended in the census data for cities and their suburbs, though the 
tenth of our people there indicated as living within ten miles of the borders of 
the large cities is impressive enough, considering its ratio of increase and its 
comprising nearly half as many people as the cities themselves, in several locali- 
ties more than the centers. It may legitimately be extended to lesser cities which 
have active suburban developments, and its definition broadened to include 
other than commuting elements. Naturally such suburban life is largely urban; 
in fact, only some 10 per cent is rural, and only a fraction of this is probably 
farming population. But the recent census indication that there are a quarter 
of a million of farming population within municipal limits would indicate that 
for the lesser cities at least, the rural suburban will be a considerable number, 
and quite largely farming population. This "No-Man's-Land" phase of the rural 
problem has often been hinted at but never explored. It probably represents 
the most intense and active rural conditions in all phases, and situations toward 
which more and more of American rural life will approximate. 

For there is an even broader and wholly legitimate phase of the rural which 
this same "sub-urban" may connote. Open country and even hamlet or village 
life weakens when subjected to contacts with urban centers within a rapidly 
increasing range of distance. Kolb's Wisconsin studies are complete evidence. 
With few exceptions for industrial "pockets" the urban places have tributary 
country and village population, whose contribution to the total community life 
is lessening relatively. One-half of all the counties have such an urban place 
within them. There are less than one-tenth of the counties that have no incor- 
porated place (generally 250 people and up), indicating that little more than 
three-quarters of a million are beyond ten miles from a village. On the other 



164 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

hand there are more than one-tenth of all the counties in which a single city con- 
tains more than half the county's population. These are almost always cities of 
more than 10,000 people, and may be presumed to exert determining influences 
over their counties. The detailed nature and variety of the suburbanizing in- 
fluences of the rural have not been studied, but their scope seems to warrant it. 

Rural sociology has been advancing steadily toward a sense of the complex- 
ity, the relations, and the gradations with its field. Where the line should be 
drawn for urban and rural is very uncertain, and even whether it is really worth 
while drawing it at all as things are coming to be, is an open question. 

The importance of this "twilight zone" makes the contribution of Fry's 
village studies all the more significant, for the preliminary report of findings 
greatly affects many phases of the treatment of Significant Factors in Rural 
Populations affecting Our Civilization and also The Vital Statistics of Rural 
Communities. 

Any treatment or analysis of rural and urban as wholes necessarily ignores 
the community concept, and is so far forth invalid. 

Rural sociology must, as Professor Gillette points out in the beginning of 
his paper, recognize, and that fully, the functional interrelationship of rural and 
urban. It may be questioned if Professor Thompson's very pointed closing in- 
dictment of the vital conditions of the cities does so. The differences between ur- 
ban and rural are fully displayed, but these are presented rather as mere condi- 
tions than as functional adjustments to life-needs alike in country and city. 
The economic and social, and therefore the vital, functions of the two spheres 
of life may be different. 

Our American cities are still largely cases of urban pioneering and hardly 
cities yet. They are what they are vitally and demographically largely because 
the country is what it is. Conceivably the cities will be better off when they 
are more largely recruited from within, as they tend more and more to be. 

Calculations of the possible effects of rural depopulation need not alarm us 
when it is seen that despite retarded production the food value of what is pro- 
duced is maintained and advanced. This result is only another evidence that 
the new agriculture, which is at its best in urban regions and constantly expand- 
ing, may be counted upon for the future. 

As usual, the lament is still made over the areas of rural decline. Yet if the 
obverse of the figures presented to maintan that tradition be taken it is readily 
seen that there are very considerable, and even more important, growing areas 
of American rural life. In the main, too, these are in these same urban or sub- 
urbanized regions above referred to. And there is sufficient evidence that in spite 
of widespread agricultural depression these areas are and have been prosperous 
to a degree far above the average. 

Rural sociology must, it would seem, get away from a preoccupation with 
rural life as it has been to the forms of it which are rapidly assuming importance 
and suggesting the forms of the near future. 

It must get onto the firing line where urban and rural are most closely re- 
lated and interpenetrating. 



SECTION ON SOCIAL RESEARCH 



CONTRIBUTIONS FOR CHARITY, RELIGION, AND EDUCA- 
TION OF THE INCOME TAXPAYERS OF DANE 
COUNTY, WISCONSIN, 1922 



JOHN L. GILLIN, UNIVERSITY OF WISCONSIN 



The Wisconsin income-tax law provides for exemptions on account of con- 
tributions to charity, education, and religion. A recent study of 27,000 state 
income-tax returns in Dane County, Wisconsin, shows 5,317 in which a definite 
contribution was listed. There were others in which only the total to these 
three purposes was indicated and hence were not used in this study. 

The total taxable income of these 5,317 taxpayers amounted to $6,709,932; 
the total contributions to $358,810. Of the contributions, 72 per cent was given 
to religion, 18 per cent to charity, and 10 per cent to educational organizations. 

Of the amount given to religion ($258,476), 92 per cent was contributed to 
the churches, and 8 per cent to other religious organizations. Of the $64,127 
given to charity, over one-third (37 per cent) was given to the hospitals, the 
remainder to the organizations listed as charitable. Thirty-six thousand, two 
hundred seven dollars, or 10 per cent of the total, was contributed to educational 
organizations. This seems a surprisingly large amount, but when it is remem- 
bered that the financial drive for the Memorial Union at the University was 
carried on in 1922, it is not surprising that 43 per cent of this amount was con- 
tributed for that purpose. Perhaps in a normal year not more than one-half this 
amount is given to educational purposes, largely parochial schools. 

The law allows a deduction of 10 per cent of one's income to contributions 
to these three objects. From the figures already given it is apparent that these 
more than 5,000 taxpayers were much less generous than the law allows, for 
they did not average more than about 5 per cent. 

A comparison of the different vocational and professional groups throws 
some interesting light upon the philanthropic interests of the respective groups. 
Do the groups with the highest taxable income contribute the most? They do 
not. Farmers who had 6 per cent of the taxable income in Dane County gave 
1 6 per cent of their income to these three purposes. Salesmen, on the other hand, 
who had 12 per cent of the total taxable income contributed only 3 per cent. 
Table I shows the relationship of the different classes. 

Contributions per person in each of these groups of occupations varies from 
$35 for students to $393 for manufacturers. This table indicates also the amount 
each member of the various occupational groups should have given had he con- 
tributed the maximum amount permitted by law exempt from taxation. In the 

165 



i66 



THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 



column beside these amounts is the average sum actually contributed by the 
individuals in this occupation. 

A comparison of the givers in the various occupations is interesting. The 
only groups which contributed more than 10 per cent of their taxable incomes 
were farmers, laborers, clergymen, and those whose occupation was unknown. 
Does that situation suggest the superior generosity of these classes? Perhaps. 
But may it not indicate that the farmers had become accustomed to a scale 

TABLE I 

COMPARISON OF TAXABLE INCOME AND DONATION FOR EACH 
OCCUPATIONAL GROUP* 



OCCUPATIONAL GROUP 


AVERAGE PER PERSON 


PER CENT 


I 

Taxable 
Income 


II 

Donations 


III 

Ten Per Cent 
of Taxable 
Income 


IV 

Taxable 
Income to 
Total Taxab 
Income 


V 

Taxable 
Income to 
Total 
Donations 


Manufacturers 


6,408 
5,107 
4,302 
1,322 
2,718 
2,509 
1,143 
2,733 
2,490 
2,078 
1,136 
1,400 

2,193 
750 
i,234 
296 
928 
779 
358 
617 
734 


393 
190 

185 
164 
144 

120 

118 

98 

91 

85 

77 
72 

59 
58 
54 
47 
44 
45 
37 
36 
35 


640 
510 
430 
132 
271 
250 
114 
273 
249 
207 

H3 
140 
219 

75 
123 

29 
92 
77 
35 
61 

73 


3 
4 
7 
I 

5 
3 
3 

2 

18 
7 

2 

3 

12 
I 

6 
6 

9 
i 
i 

5 

i 


6 
6 
4 

12 

5 

5 

10 

4 
4 
4 
7 
5 
3 
7 
4 
16 

6 

10 

6 
5 


Lawyers 


Professors . . . 


Clergymen 


Doctors 


Bankers 


Unknown 


Public officials 


Merchants 


Other professions 


Retired 


People in service to public 
Salesmen 


Housekeepers 


Teachers 


Farmers 


Mechanics 


Public-utility employees. . 
Laborers 


Office employees 


Students 





* Based on Wisconsin income-tax returns for 1922, for Dane County. 

of giving when times were good which they continued for social reasons when 
they could not so well afford it? 

If, however, one takes into account the ratio of the total donation of the 
class to the taxable income of the class, fanners stand at the top and salesmen 
at the bottom. 

The percentages of taxable income given by the different occupational 
groups was as follows: fanners, 16; clergymen, 12; unknown, 10; laborers, 10; 
retired, 7; housekeepers, 7; lawyers, 6; office employees, 6; public-utility em- 
ployees, 6; manufacturers, 6; doctors, 5; people in service to public, 5; students, 
5; bankers, 5; mechanics, 5; professors, 4; teachers, 4; other professions, 4 
merchants, 4; public officials, 4; salesmen, 3. 



CONTRIBUTIONS OF INCOME TAXPAYERS 167 

Manufacturers, clergymen, lawyers, and those whose occupations are un- 
known are the only groups which gave above the mean for both actual amounts 
and percentages of gifts to taxable income. 

Those who gave more than the average 5 per cent of their incomes were: 
farmers, 16 per cent; clergymen, 12 per cent; unknown, 10 per cent; laborers, 
10 per cent; housekeepers, 7 per cent; retired, 7 per cent; lawyers, 6 per cent; 
office employees, 6 per cent; public utility employees, 6 per cent; manufacturers, 
6 per cent. Those who gave less than this average of 5 per cent were: pro- 
fessors, 4 per cent; teachers, 4 per cent; other professions, 4 per cent; merchants, 
4 per cent; public officials, 4 per cent; salesmen, 3 per cent. 

Doctors, people in the service of the public, students, bankers, and mech- 
anics gave just the average of 5 per cent. 

Is this study at all significant of the philanthropic spirit of people in the 
various occupations? On the face of the returns, farmers, clergymen, and those 
of unknown occupations, and laborers gave 10 per cent or more of their taxable 
incomes; retired individuals and housekeepers, 7 per cent; lawyers, office em- 
ployees, public utility employees, and manufacturers, 6 per cent; doctors, peo- 
ple in the service of the public, students, bankers, and mechanics, 5 per cent; 
professors, teachers, those in other professions, merchants, and public officials, 
4 per cent, while salesmen trail along with 3 per cent. This looks like a descend- 
ing scale of generosity. Probably it is not a just measure of either generosity or 
stinginess. Standards of living, the mores of each group, and demands made 
upon some groups not deductable under the law, such as support of dependents, 
expenditures for self-improvement, etc., would have to be considered in this 
connection. However, the figures do make clear that there are wide discrep- 
ancies in actual practice of giving. 

It is clear from these figures that religion commands much larger contribu- 
tions than education or charity. That is easy to explain in the case of the farmer, 
since the religious institution is the only one making any large appeal in the 
country. Professors in the university gave the lowest percentage to religion. 
Bankers, doctors, lawyers, and public officials gave a much lower percentage 
than did laborers, mechanics, public utility employees, and students. 

Charity ranks ahead of education in these gifts. Exceptions to this were 
to be found in the case of professors, public officials, students, and those whose 
occupations are unknown. The drive for the Memorial Union at the University 
would explain this. Naturally doctors gave the largest share to hospitals. It 
is quite plain, however, that in spite of these exceptions, the church as the oldest 
of these institutions commands more of the financial support of these taxpayers 
than all others combined. Out of a total of $358,810, the church got $237,029, 
while all other religious activities got from this group only $21,447. 

In summary, four times as much was given to religion as to charity, and 
nearly twice as much to charity as to education. These more than 5,000 tax- 
payers gave on the average about 5 per cent of their taxable incomes to these 
philanthropic purposes. 



1 68 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

THE CHILDREN OF THE POOR: SOME STATISTICAL 
OBSERVATIONS IN DAVENPORT, IOWA 



HORNELL HART, Bryn Mawr College 



Social workers have long known that children from poor families differ 
markedly from children of normal families in their intelligence, school progress, 
energy, age of leaving school, and size of family from which they come. These 
matters of common knowledge have, however, rarely or never been reduced to 
a definite basis by comparing accurately the children in families known to be 
dependent with children in other families. The Iowa Child Welfare Research 
Station recently made an investigation in Davenport, Iowa, with a view to 
determining the relationship between size of family and certain mental and 
social characteristics of the children of those families. A total of 589 families 
having children born in the year 1908 who were attending public schools in 
Davenport in December, 1923, were investigated. Mental tests were given to 
the children and information was secured as to their school progress, and their 
elimination from school, and teachers' opinions as to certain of their character 
traits. 

Search was then made, through the records of the confidential exchange, 
juvenile court, school dental clinic, and indigent book list for 1923, to find what 
families had received charitable relief. When the confidential exchange showed 
that a family from the group studied had been on the records of the county poor 
relief or of the leading private charity organization (the Ladies' Industrial Relief 
Society) the original records of these agencies were consulted. The families 
were then classified into four groups: (i) independent families, who had not 
appeared during the periods covered in the records of any of these relief agencies; 
(2) occasionally dependent families, who appeared only once and in the records 
of only one agency; (3) intermittently dependent families, who had appeared on 
the records of two or more agencies, but who had not more than one contact 
with either the county or Ladies' Industrial Relief agencies; (4) chronically 
dependent families who had more repeated relief contacts than the above groups. 
Of the 589 families studied, 23, or 3.9 per cent, were chronic dependents; 23, or 
3.9 per cent, were intermittent dependents; 69, or 11.7 per cent, were occasional 
dependents; and 474, or 80.5 per cent, were independent. Table III summarizes 
the relationship between poverty and size of family. 

Chronic dependency is over four times as great among families with seven 
or more children as it is in smaller families. (Data on this point are summarized 
in Table III.) In addition to having numerous brothers and sisters, children 
from poor families tend to be stupid, retarded, and lazy, and tend to leave school 
early. Data on these points for the children from the dependency groups are 
shown in Table IV. 

The difference in average mental-test quotients of children from dependent 
and independent families is 8.9^1.65, leaving no doubt as to the relationship 



THE CHILDREN OF THE POOR 



169 



between poverty and mental-test inferiority. Of the children tested who had 
quotients of no or over, only 8.1 2. 2 per cent came from dependent families; 
of the children who had quotients of less than 90, the percentage from dependent 
families is 44. 2 2.0. 

Similarly striking is the relationship of poverty and retardation. The age- 
grade residuals of children from the chronically dependent families are 6.35^.84 

TABLE I 



Number of Living Children 
per Family 


Percentage of 1908 
Children Attending 
Part-Time School 


Difference from 
Families with One 
and Two Children 


One and two 


3. 0=*= I. 3 




Three and four 


8 0=*=! 4 


e 6=*= 2 O 


Five and six 


i6.9=*=2. i 


I3.9=t=2.5 


Seven and eight . 


2C 0=*= 2. Q 


22. 0=^3 2 


Nine and over 


y? 2d= -i n 


7Q ^^/t I 








All sizes 


IO (X 











TABLE II 



Mental-Test Quotient 



Percentage of 1908 Children 
in Part-Time School 



1 10144 


2. I =*=!. 7 


90-109 


8. *=*=!. I 


65-89 


21. 2=*=1.8 






Age-Grade Residuals* 




IO 34. ... 


I 8==2 7 


o o . . 


7 2=*=I 2 


10 I 


O. 7=*=I. 3 


30 ii 


2Q. 2=*= 2. 







* The "age-grade residual" is an index of school progress 
relative to chronological and mental age. A high age-grade re- 
sidual means that the child is farther along in school than might 
have been expected from his mental development; a low residual 
means a greater degree of retardation than the child's mental 
age justifies. 

lower than those of children from independent families. This means an average 
difference of over half a grade in school progress at the age of sixteen, in addition 
to differences related to mental-test ability. The intermittently and occasionally 
dependent families have children intermediate between the two groups. 

The percentage of children leaving school prematurely is 3 7. 2 4.4 larger in 
chronically dependent families than in independent families. 

Children from independent families average io.4i.o in their energy 
ratings, as compared with i.irfci.Q for all dependents and io.i4.i for 
children from chronically dependent families. 



TEE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 



The average number of living children is 2.o8db.32 larger in chronically 
dependent families than in independent. This means that the chronic depend- 
ents are maintaining a net reproductive rate about 60 per cent higher than the 
independent families. 

TABLE III 
POVERTY IN RELATION TO SIZE OF FAMILY 



NUMBER OF LIVING 
CHILDREN IN FAMILY 


PERCENTAGE OF FAMILIES 


NUMBER OF 
FAMILIES 


Chronically 
Dependent 


Independent 


I 


0.0=*=1.5 

a-r^i-o 

I. !=*=!. 3 

S-9^1-3 
i.6==i.6 
3.8=4=2.6 

3.1=1=0.6 
14- 1*1. 5 


95.7*3-2 
84.0=^=2. i 
84.0=1=2.7 
77.5=4=2.6 
81.0=1=3.4 
73.0=4=5.2 

83.3*1.2 

6O.6=4=3. 2 


71 
162 

94 

102 

63 
26 

518 
71 


2 


2 




4. 
tj 


6 


6 and under 


7 and over . 


Difference 


ii.o==i.6 


22.73.4 




All sizes 




3-9^0.5 


8o.5=4=I.I 


589 





TABLE IV 

POVERTY IN RELATIONSHIP TO MENTAL TEST ABILITY, RETARDATION, ELIMINATION 
FROM SCHOOL, ENERGY RATINGS, AND SIZE OF FAMILY 



DEPENDENCY 
STATUS 


FAMILIES 


AVERAGE 
MENTAL- 
TEST 
QUOTIENTS 


AVERAGE 
AGE-GRADE 
RESIDUALS 


PERCENT- 
AGE OF 
1908 CHIL- 
DREN IN 
PART- 
TIME 
SCHOOLS 


AVERAGE 
ENERGY 
RATINGS 


AVERAGE 
NUMBER 
OF LIVING 
CHILDREN 

PER 

FAMILY 


Num- 
ber 


Per- 
cent- 
age 


Chronic dependents 
Intermittent dependents . 
Occasional dependents. . . 

All dependents 
Independent families 

All families . ... 


23 

8 


3-9 
3 9 
xx. 7 


92.6*1.14 

88.4*1 22 

94 1*0.77 


- s<7 6 =b. 82 
2 09=*=. 85 
-o 38=*= 54 


43- 5*4 3 

21 7*4-3 

21 7*2 5 


IO 1*4 I 

5-5 sfc 44 
4 0*2 6 


5- 48*. 31 
5-13=*= 31 
4 i6*.i8 


us 

474 


19-5 
80.5 


92.5*0.57 
101 .4*0.31 


i. 77* .40 
.59=*=- 22 


26.1 * I .9 

6 3 o.8 


1.1*1.9 
10,4*1 o 


4.62* 14 
3. 40*. 07 


589 


100. 

























In addition to these facts as to the feeblemindedness, laziness, retardation, 
and fecundity of poor families, the Davenport study brought out some very 
striking results relative to conditions associated with early departure from 
school. The Iowa law permits children fourteen to sixteen years of age to secure 
work permits under certain conditions, but such children are required to attend 
part-time schools on two half-days per week. Enrolment in these part-time 
classes, therefore, is an indication of early elimination from school. The Daven- 



CHANGES IN CUSTOMS AND BELIEFS OF MODERN JEWS 171 

port study shows strikingly the relationship between school elimination, pov- 
erty, size of family, mental-test ability, and retardation. The relationship with 
dependency has been shown in Table IV; the other three variables are discussed 
below. 

The percentages of children born in 1908 who were attending part-time 
school were, for various sizes of families, as indicated in Table I. 

Of children born in 1908, from families of five or more living children, 22.0 
per cent attended part-time school; of those from families with four children, 
1 1.8 were in part-time school. Elimination of larger families might, therefore, be 
expected to cut down premature elimination from school by nearly as much as a 
half. 

In addition to the correlations with poverty and with size of family, early 
elimination from school is correlated with low mental-test ability and tow age- 
grade residuals. The comparisons in Table II are significant. 

Among children in families with five or more living children, those who 
were first to fifth born had 12.1 1.5 per cent in part-time schools, while those 
sixth to tenth born had 10.9 2. 3 per cent. While the difference of 1.2 2. 7 is 
trivial, it is interesting to note that it runs in the opposite direction from what 
would be expected in view of the fact that higher birth orders are in the larger 
families. 

The complete results of the Davenport survey, from which the materials 
for this note are taken, are in the hands of the Iowa Child Welfare Research 
Station at Iowa City, Iowa. 



RELATIVE RATE OF CHANGE IN CUSTOMS AND BELIEFS 
OF MODERN JEWS 1 



JESSIE RAVITCH, UNIVERSITY OF MINNESOTA 



The purpose of this study was to apply as far as possible objective and 
quantitative methods to the analysis of the intangible psycho-social environ- 
ment 2 as they are applied in the study of the physical and chemical environ- 
ments. In other words, to state in mathematical terms psycho-social relation- 
ships, as physical and chemical relationships are. 

All scientifically induced laws are stated in terms of a hypothetical norm, 
and variations are computed on this basis. In sociology, however, because the 

1 The study of which this paper is an abstract was made under the direction of 
Professor L. L. Bernard in connection with his seminar in social theory. It was awarded 
the Menorah Prize of $100 donated by Mr. Arthur Harris, of Minneapolis, Minnesota. 

3 For a detailed explanation of the concept of the psycho-social environment used 
in this investigation, see Professor Bernard's paper on the classification of environments 
to appear in the September, 1925, American Journal of Sociology. 



172 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

data are so varied and complex, it has been impossible to abstract as yet such a 
norm; the induction of laws, therefore, has been greatly restricted. 

In the present investigation this difficulty was overcome by the selection of 
Jews as subjects of study. The psycho-social environment which they have ac- 
cumulated during their history furnishes an excellent basis from which to meas- 
ure change. That is, it furnishes a constant from which variations can be com- 
puted objectively. It contains hundreds of commands, precepts, customs, tradi- 
tions, and other psycho-social elements to which the hypothetically perfect Jew 
must conform. This does not necessarily mean that any Jews exist who do ad- 
here to all these dictates, any more than there are any objects that actually fall 
according to the law of falling bodies. The cases are exactly analogous. The 
basis from which change in Jews was measured in this study, their accumulated 
psycho-social environment, corresponds to the basis from which the velocity of 
all falling bodies is computed, namely, a vacuum. But in this problem not every 
item of the Jewish psycho-social environment could be used, since it would have 
involved hundreds of questions. Only key items were chosen, therefore; those 
which were considered most important from the standpoint of change. These 
consisted of (a) six outstanding customs: (i) observance of the Sabbath; (2) 
observance of the Passover; (3) observance of Rosh Hashonah; (4) observance 
of Yom Kippur; (5) observance of the dietary laws outside of the home; (6) 
fasting; and (b) five significant beliefs: (i) belief in the Deity; (2) belief in after- 
life; (3) belief in Zionism; (4) belief in evolution; (5) attitude toward inter- 
marriage. 

Two miscellaneous reactions were also included as interesting in themselves 
as well as checks upon the results obtained. They were: (i) whether or not the 
individual had ever wished he were not a Jew, and (2) whether or not he had 
ever denied the fact that he was a Jew. 

The individual who observed all the customs, believed in the Deity, in 
after-life, and Zionism, was opposed to intermarriage and the theory of evolu- 
tion, who had never wished he was not a Jew nor ever denied it, was considered 
as having not changed at all. This, of course, was not strictly accurate, since 
he may have changed in other customs and beliefs which were not included in 
this study. But since these reactions are significant ones, they do serve as in- 
dexes of change. 

So much for the base from which change was measured. But before I pass 
on to the other data, I wish to emphasize the point that no ethical, moral, or 
evaluative connotations are implied in the term change. A man who had 
changed in 50 to 75 per cent of the items might consider himself as good a Jew as 
one who had changed less than 10 per cent. 

The degree of change in itself, however, would be of no great value unless 
we knew the conditions under which it took place. To return to the analogy of 
the law of falling bodies, whenever the physicist wishes to know the velocity 
with which an object is going to fall, he must know the conditions of the environ- 
ment in which the object is to fall. In this study, therefore, the environmental 



CHANGES IN CUSTOMS AND BELIEFS OF MODERN JEWS 173 

conditions in which the change occurred were also investigated. The following 
environmental categories were used: 

I. Physical factors of the environment 
i. Geographical 

a) Birthplace of self and parents (nationality) 

b) Present residence 
II. Biological factors 

1. Age 

2. Sex 

III. "Psychological" factors (for lack of a better term) 

1. Percentage of life spent in the United States 

2. Membership in non- Jewish organizations 

3. Reading of non- Jewish magazines regularly 

4. Language habits 

a) Read Yiddish and Hebrew 

b) Speak Yiddish and Hebrew 

c) Understand Yiddish and Hebrew 

5. Whether or not the individual had ever been the object of anti-Semitic feeling 

IV. Social 

1. Economic 

a) Occupation 

b) Income 

c) Financial status of offspring, i.e., 

(1) Wholly self-supporting 

(2) Partially self-supporting 

(3) Dependent on parents 

(4) Helping support the family 

2. Education 

a) Secular 

b) Religious 

(1) Talmud Torah 

(2) Sunday school 

(3) Private rabbi 

3. Familial and home conditions 

a) Father living 

b) Mother living 

c) Grandparents living 

d) Language spoken in the home 

e) Home kept kosher 

These, then, were the data used. The method employed in collecting them 
was a combination of questionnaire and schedule; that is, it combined the con- 
ciseness of the questionnaire with the completeness of the schedule. It contained 
about ninety items including the material listed above. Twelve hundred of 
these were distributed through organizations connected with various Jewish 
institutions of Minneapolis, and a few were sent to New York and St. Paul. 
Of these, 369, or approximately 33 per cent were returned. The answers were 



174 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

tabulated as yes, no, vague, and blank. Only the categorical noes (in two cases 
yeses) were taken to mean change. The results give therefore a minimum, rather 
than a maximum, degree of change, since many of the vague answers might have 
been interpretated as negative. For total change of custom observance, all the 
noes were added and divided by six, because six customs were used; for total 
change in belief the totals were divided by five. Belief in the Deity and observ- 
ance of the dietary laws as the most fundamental indexes were carried through 
the environmental categories separately and compared with the synthetic 
results. 

The detailed results, amounting to several hundred in number, may be 
reduced to the following conclusions: 

1. The total change was 25.9 per cent. This might mean either that all Jews 
had changed this much, or that 25.9 per cent had changed completely, or any- 
thing between these extremes. 1 

2. Beliefs showed a tendency to change before custom observance, thus 
bearing out the generalization of Ross to this effect. 

3. Offspring change more than parents. 

4. Native-born tend to change more than foreign-born. 

5. Observance of customs varies inversely as the frequency with which they 
must be practiced. 

6. Dietary customs change more than non-dietary customs of the same 
relative frequency of occurrence. 

7. Yom Kippur is the most strongly adhered to of the customs. 

8. Belief in the Deity is the most strongly adhered to of the beliefs. 

9. German Jews (Reform Jews) change more than Jews of other nativity 
or extraction, and more than third-generation Jews, who are second in order. 
Russian and Roumanian Jews show equal change. 

10. The greatest degree of change in Minneapolis is found in the lake dis- 
trict, next greatest in the south district, and least change in the north district. 
This is in inverse order to the density of Jewish population and order of Jewish 
settlement. 

11. Sex as such has little to do with change aside from the environmental 
factors which operate to produce change. 

12. At the present time the peak for change occurs between the ages of 
twenty-five and twenty-nine. 

13. There is a slight positive correlation 3 between increasing change and the 
percentage of life spent in this country. 

14. Belonging to non- Jewish organizations is correlated 2 with increased 
change. 

1 It is interesting to note that this figure is close to the mean of various non-statis- 
tical estimates of change. These estimates have ranged from the conservative guess of 
10 per cent to Zangwill's guess of 33 per cent. 

2 The term correlation is here used in a non-technical sense. 



CHANGES IN CUSTOMS AND BELIEFS OF MODERN JEWS 175 

15. Reading non- Jewish magazines regularly is correlated 1 with trend to- 
ward radicalism or change. 

1 6. Ability to read Yiddish is not a marked factor in conservatism, or lack 
of change; ability to speak and understand it are factors in this connection. 

17. Mild anti-Semitic feeling is not an influence toward conservatism; 
severe anti-Semitism is. 

1 8. Occupations show a correlation 1 between order of change and the 
amount of formal training necessary for their pursuance; i.e., professional, 
clerical, students, salesmen, entrepreneurs, and housewives. 

19. There is a very slight secular trend toward increased change with in- 
creased income. 

20. There is a slight correlation 1 between degree of financial independence of 
offspring and change in outward practices. 

21. There is a marked positive correlation 1 between number of years of 
secular education and degree of change. 

22. There is a marked negative correlation 1 between number of years of 
religious education and change in overt practice. 

23. Talmud-Torah training is the most effective type of Jewish training. 

24. Living parents act as a conservative influence. 

25. Mother and father are of practically equal importance in this respect; 
they are more influential in the matter of practices than in beliefs. 

26. There is a slight correlation 1 between the existence of grandparents and 
conservatism. 

27. Those who come from homes where Yiddish is spoken show less change 
than those who do not. 

28. Those who come from Kosher homes change less than those who do not. 

29. Beliefs are less standardized than customs. 

30. Face-to-face contacts influence customs more than beliefs. 

31. Non-face-to-face, or derivative contacts, influence beliefs more than 
customs. 

32. Offspring differ from parents more in beliefs than in customs. 

33. Foreign-born differ from native-born more in belief than in custom 
observance. 

These conclusions may be further abstracted into the following generaliza- 
tions which are of more direct importance for social psychology : 

1. Beliefs show less uniformity than custom observance, and they tend to 
change before custom as well. 

2. Language habits are influential in behavior; that is, we tend to act 
according to the standards of the psycho-social environment of which our 
dominant language habits are symbols. 

3. Relative degree of observance of customs varies inversely with the fre- 

1 The term correlation is here used in a non-technical sense. 



176 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

quency with which they must be practiced, and those which interfere with eco- 
nomic adjustment are discarded first. 

4. Sex as such has little to do with promoting or retarding change. 

5. Mild or trivial persecution of a socially inferior class by the socially 
superior accelerates the degree of imitation of the superior by the inferior; 
severe persecution retards it. 

6. Primary or face-to-face contacts influence practices more than beliefs; 
derivitive or distance contacts influence beliefs more than practices. 



PERSONALITY AND LIFE-HISTORY DOCUMENTS 



E. T. KRUEGER, VANDERBILT UNIVERSITY 



This research is based upon documents of personal experience. A life- 
history document, in the sense in which it is here used, is the record of a person, 
written by himself, in which his own life is the principle theme. 

While a long list of terms has come into existence in literature to designate 
the effort to record the self, such as life, journal, memoir, autobiography, reflec- 
tions, diary, letters, and chronicle, these can be reduced to three general types, 
the life, the diary, and letters. 

This research has not been made upon extant literary documents but upon 
fresh materials, personally secured. 

Life-history documents have not been used to any great extent for social 
research. Thomas and Znaniecki in the Polish Peasant, Thomas in The Unad- 
justed Girl, and Park and Miller in Old World Traits Transplanted indicate some 
of the pioneers in this field. Nor has any significant study been made of the 
documents themselves and their validity for scientific purposes. Several literary 
studies have been made, such as Anna Robeson Burrs's The Autobiography, and 
Arthur Ponsonby's English Diaries. This research project has sought to test 
out the usefulness and validity of personal documents for scientific purposes. 
It is therefore a research primarily in the method of the case study. 

In general there are two main types of documents, One is introspective and 
reflective, revealing the inner private life in terms of the fundamental motives or 
attitudes and the social situations which call these attitudes into existence. The 
other is the conventional document, which is highly rationalized and idealized, 
and which deals mainly with external and traditional behavior in terms of social 
approval. 

Both introspective and conventional documents are valid sociological data. 
They are behavior in the same sense as any reaction (as in physics, chemistry, 
or physiology) is behavior. The technique for the use and comparative handling 
of these materials is alone lacking. 

Introspective and conventional documents may be further classified into 



PERSONALITY AND LIFE-HISTORY DOCUMENTS 177 

four types, based upon what might be called the autobiographical motive, the 
confessional, the egotistical, the scientific, and the naive type. 

a) The confessional document is based upon the attitude of inferiority, of 
defeat, and failure incident to personal disorganization. It is always intro- 
spective in character. 

b) Egotistical documents may be either introspective or conventional. The 
attitudes are those of superiority. The documents show a high degree of self- 
appreciation and idealization of r61e. Three subtypes compose this general type: 
those which reveal a defensive assumption of superiority, as in Rousseau's Con- 
fessions; those in which the attitudes are conventionalized and rationalized, as 
in The Americanization of Edward Bok, and those which are products of eman- 
cipation and detachment, as in Pepys' Diary. 

c) There is a third general group which can be called scientific. These docu- 
ments are always introspective and represent a deliberate purpose to portray 
life objectively and in terms of scientific analysis. They are devoid of attitudes 
of superiority or inferiority. They make no emotional response to social values, 
except to record the reponses which once took place. 

d) A fourth type may be called the naive document. It is highly conven- 
tional and represents experience in terms of persons who are submerged beneath 
a relatively fixed group-culture, and who have no essentially private life. 

The foregoing classification isolates the confessional document, which forms 
the chief interest and contribution of this research. 

What interests us in the confessional document is that the mechanism of 
catharsis or release from tension which underlies it is operative in crisis or 
tension situations. When catharsis occurs the effect is a pouring out of the men- 
tal jam incident to personal disorganization. When socially forbidden experi- 
ences become dammed up in the mind as objects of mental conflict, when defeat 
and failure in the realization of wishes face the person, when habits and senti- 
ments are deeply disturbed, confession as a means of relief can take place. It 
is a natural outlet for mental tension. It is highly erroneous to think that per- 
sons will not tell their private lives. When persons are disorganized and strug- 
gling for reorganization, self-revelation is a natural phenomenon. Catharsis is 
a response to the necessity of readjustment of the personality to social situations. 
Disorganization is to be understood in relation to reorganization. Penitential 
prayer and religious confession are cases in point. 

The value of documents of the private life, and more especially of the con- 
fessional type of private-life document, lies in the use of these documents as a 
source for the study of attitudes. Human behavior, to be understood, must be 
studied from the standpoint of the person's own attitudes toward his experi- 
ences. Our efforts to be scientific have resulted sometimes in seeking to impose 
upon another's experience our reactions to his experience. The facts in any 
experience are the attitudes directed toward that experience by the person hav- 
ing it. Behavior is conditioned and motivated by the person's attitude toward 
himself and his r61e. 



178 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

When situations are of the nature of a crisis, when adjustments to situations 
are difficult, and personal disorganization impends, tension or mental conflict 
arises. In severe cases the blocking may be complete. It is not too much to 
suggest, as the result of this research, that the confessional document as herein 
defined, whether in the form of a life-history, a diary, or of letters, offers a highly 
satisfactory and thus far an almost undiscovered form of data by which person- 
ality and social problems may become explicable. 

For the description of the private life in terms of fundamental motives and 
attitudes no other form of data is at present available except some form of 
introspection. The confessional document is a form of introspective narrative 
which has especial values for scientific purposes. The problem of the private life 
is a problem of the imagination, and the measurement of the imagination does 
not now appear to be a problem in physiological behaviorism, but a problem of 
attitudes. In the confessional document these attitudes lie revealed. 

It is a conclusion of this research that the confessional document, as does 
no other form of data, provides an intimate case-study of the private life; that 
as a response to the mechanism of tension and catharsis it reveals the funda- 
mental motives or attitudes which underlie behavior. 

Criticism of the use of personal documents as case studies tends to question 
the reliability of the documents. Pathological lying occurs in response to de- 
fense or self-approval. The confessional document is not defensive in character 
and the attitudes are the opposite of self-approval. The facts disclosed in a 
confessional document are such as to invite disparagement rather than approval. 
And essentially the writer seeks relief from the burden of inner conflict. 

Our position here, however, is that every document is data, the pathological 
liar product as well as the confessional document. The former does not fall with- 
in the latter class, but is in itself highly significant and of the utmost value in 
the analysis of personality. 

The merit of the confessional document "for scientific purposes lies in the 
following: (i) A high degree of candor. This insures what in common-sense 
terms may be called truthfulness. The confession is hot and eruptive. It is not 
defensive. (2) Completeness of detail. The person writing is driven to write 
comprehensively and elaborately. In the effort to make others see as he sees 
and feel as he feels, completeness is a natural result. (3) A minimum of self- 
idealization. The genius of the confessional document is that it is self-disparag- 
ing. (4) A revelation of the fundamental motives or attitudes. These are ex- 
pressed in the general cartharsis, in which the mental content set up by reflection 
and brooding upon experience is set loose. 

After a fundamental classification was secured which isolated the confes- 
sional document, a technique was developed to secure life-history documents 
from persons experiencing disorganization. The limits of this paper do not 
permit a description of this technique. It is enough to indicate that the con- 
fessional document is sufficiently typical so that a technique has been developed 
to call it into existence. 



PERSONALITY AND LIFE-HISTORY DOCUMENTS 179 

The results of this research may be stated further in terms of the uses to 
which life-history documents may be put. 

In the study of any particular personality case, a life-history document 
gives a connected account of the life. The result is a total picture of the per- 
sonality, on the one hand, and on the other hand, a detailed and genetic account 
of the situations and attitudes which constitute the life-experience. As in Healy's 
cases of the individual delinquent, this permits an intelligent appreciation of the 
forces operating in any particular person's experience, but from the sociological 
point of view rather than the psychological. 

Life-history documents can, in and of themselves, be used comparatively. 
We have seen that the confessional document rises in response to situations 
which cause mental tension. These may be called tension or crisis situations. 
In that they enforce adjustments affecting life-organization they play a role in 
the fixation of attitudes which determine personality. They may well be called 
personality-making situations. 

The following classification of situations which cause mental tensions is 
quite tentative, but represents an effort to utilize the documents comparatively. 
Control and prediction in personality hangs upon our ability to secure such 
classifications. This study has secured the following list of tension situations: 
health, mental ability, economic, vocation, affection, sex, personal attractive- 
ness, religious belief, cultural heritage, status, and family. These classes seem 
mutually exclusive, but probably will require both revised and additional terms. 

It is characteristic of tension situations to create attitudes. The persistence 
of mental conflict may result in the fixation of typical reaction patterns. Ten- 
sions tend to become cumulative in their effect. In fact, severe tension can 
probably never be wholly overcome. It remains latent in persons who succeed 
in readjusting themselves after a period of personal disorganization. Where the 
tension has been cumulative, life-history documents reveal fixed reaction patterns 
in terms of attitudes of inferiority. These may be thought of as mechanisms of 
inferiority. They can be stated in terms of the series a, b, c, d, e, in which (a) is a 
tension situation, (b) a resulting mental conflict, (c) a feeling of isolation, (d) a 
sense of inferiority, and (e) a behavior reaction pattern. The various behavior 
reaction patterns thus far secured are struggle, rebellion, withdrawal, and sub- 
mission. While they are called behavior reaction patterns, they are in some sense 
also types of personality. 

Life-history documents are rich in descriptions of groups which condition 
and control behavior. The r61e of the group in personal life-organization con- 
stitutes a chief contribution of sociology to the explanation of human behavior. 
Our thesis is that in life-history documents one may find the attitudes which 
result from the interplay of the person with other persons and with various 
types of group organization. This is particularly striking in the study of the 
family. 

It is suggested that the life-history gives a cross-section of a particular 
group or a cultural area. For example, the life-history is usually also a family 



i8o THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

history. The person cannot separate his personality from the groups which have 
aided or checked the satisfaction of his wishes. The slum and other natural cul- 
tural areas may be in part studied in the life-histories of persons who live in 
them. Cultural areas define the limits of life-organization of persons, and in- 
evitably affect the organization of attitudes constituting personality. 

Vocation, race prejudice, immigrant attitudes, and similar problems in- 
volving the satisfaction of wishes and the organization of the personal life can 
be studied through life-history documents. As a source for the fundamental 
attitudes which underlie all social problems and particularly of personality, these 
documents promise a fruitful source of future investigation and research. 



HISTORY TEXTBOOKS AND INTERNATIONAL DIFFERENCES 



DONALD R. TAFT, WELLS COLLEGE 



I am reporting upon an incomplete bit of research. My conclusions are 
therefore tentative. The problem is to demonstrate the importance of history 
textbooks as forces molding a public opinion hostile to certain countries. 

Textbooks are recognized as but one of such war-making forces. They are, 
however, to some extent, indexes of other similar forces general educational 
policies, for example which are less easily measured objectively. 

Eventually the study aims to include the texts of representative countries 
the world over. Only books now in use in secondary and upper-elementary 
schools are included. At present I can only report on thirty-two American texts, 
ten German, nine French, and a few Mexican. Even these collections are not 
yet entirely complete. 

The method has been to select critical and controversial events in history 
and compare in parallel columns the treatment of the same event in the texts 
of two or more countries concerned. For example, to compare the treatment of 
the Monroe Doctrine in South American and Mexican books on the one hand, 
and in our own books on the other. The comparisons are of two sorts: (i) com- 
parisons between brief summaries giving the point of view of the books; and 
(2) comparisons between more extended quotations. The quotations will enable 
critics to judge of the fairness of the summaries. The method is objective. No 
attempt is made to show which of two sets of texts is the more truthful. That 
is the historian's task. The only aim is to demonstrate that the next generation, 
if taught exactly opposite facts, cannot grow up with similar attitudes. Unless 
other influences intervene, hostile attitudes must develop among them. 

I have only time to present two comparisons with reference to the treatment 
of certain aspects of the European war. The French-German comparison is 
before you on four charts. Though taken from two of the more striking texts, 
it only slightly exaggerates the tone of the majority. Shall I read a part of these 
charts, reading across? 



HISTORY TEXTS AND INTERNATIONAL DIFFERENCES 181 

As to the causes of the war, French children read: The war was caused solely 
by German aggression. The Germans have long believed they were a superior 
race, and have long plotted to destroy the French. German children read: 
Germany is guiltless. The war had a number of causes, but English jealousy and 
French desire for revenge were the most important. 

As to peace efforts at the beginning of the war, French children read: 
France has always been pacific, and at the beginning of the war she did all in her 
power to keep the peace, but Germany thwarted her. German children read: 
Germany made every effort to localize the Austro-Serbian dispute, but the 
enemy nations openly incited their peoples to war. 

As to Belgian neutrality, French children read: Germany basely broke the 
Belgian neutrality treaty. German children read: English and Belgian officers 
planned the march through Belgium as early as 1908. 

As to atrocities, French children read: Germans, and Germans alone, were 
guilty of the vilest atrocities during the war. German children read: The Ger- 
man activities in Belgium were justified. The English abused German residents 
in England, and tried to starve Germany with the blockade. After the armistice 
the French abused German residents in Alsace-Lorraine. The Allies murdered 
German children by depriving Germany of cows. 

As to Alsace-Lorraine, French children read: The return of Alsace-Lorraine 
was an act of historical justice, and no plebicite was necessary. German children 
read: Alsace-Lorraine is only 13 per cent French, and its possession gives France 
the keys to Germany. 

As to the Peace Treaty, French children read: The peace is a peace of 
justice, whereas the Germans planned for us a peace of slavery. The Fourteen 
Points are the basis of the peace. German children read: The peace is a peace 
of enslavement and is a gross breach of the Allies' pre-armistice promises. 

As to the future, French children read: The Germans are beasts and the 
German menace will never cease. Therefore, France, beware ! German children 
read: German youth, this treaty must not be permanent! 

But quotations are more striking than these cold summaries. Here are two, 
one French, and one German: 

(German, at Versailles) 

Clemenceau had now reached the goal of his life. He now could unchain his hatred 
and revenge against defeated Germany. What concern of his were Wilson's Fourteen 
Points! "The Fourteen Points are a few too many," he sneered, "the dear God himself 
had only ten." He wished to set up the overlordship of France in Europe, and to deny 
Lloyd George German trade. Wilson had a difficult task against them. Often enough 
he threatened his departure. But finally Clemenceau and Lloyd George wrested his 

Fourteen Points from him, one after the other A cry of horror broke from the 

German people who were weakened by the hunger blockade. 

(French) 

The so-called German democracy is only a fiction. Only her facade has changed. 
This democratic and socialistic camouflage only warns us: "Let us in our turn keep our 



i82 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

powder dry." Adversity will without doubt force Germany to transform herself, but 

we can never forget the evil that she has done us Up to this time Germany has 

never been for France other than a cheat who deceives us; a brute who pillages and 

kills The great epoch through which we have been living is in sum only an 

epoch of the eternal duel of Ahriman against Ormudz, of Satan against God, of beastial- 

ity against humanity The savages on the other side of the Rhine have always 

menaced us. 

The American books are in general more liberal than the European I have 
examined, whether French or German. But with one or two possible exceptions 
they all tend to create attitudes of extreme hostility toward Germany. I present 
a comparison between two extreme types. It must be understood, however, that 
the thirty-two texts are mostly of the type presented first the non-liberal type. 
Long's book, used as the liberal example, is most exceptional. A half-dozen texts, 
perhaps, should be classed as intermediate between these two extreme types. 

With reference to the causes of the war, American children read in non- 
liberal books such passages as the following: "That the war was made in 
Germany is not a matter of dispute; it is a fact" (Elson). Germany "was fight- 
ing for the domination of Europe and eventually of America" (Woodburn and 
Moran). The war was the last in a series of events in which Germany had been 
constantly working against our country (Stephenson). 

The same books either disregard the Fourteen Point agreement altogether 
or give the impression that the treaty was based upon it. Thus Thwaites and 
Kendall merely say that the terms of peace were the "outgrowth of the Fourteen 
Points as modified by discussion." 

The two books classed as liberal are represented on the second American 
chart. As to the causes of the war, American children read: Germany appears 
guilty, but her plot was not a thing peculiar to her. Other nations had had plans 
of conquest. The real evil was the European system. And it is impossible yet 
to apportion accurately the blame for the war. The same book says of the 
treaty: The Allies failed to keep the Fourteen Point agreement which included 
"impartial justice with no discrimination between victors and vanquished." 
And again (speaking of the results of the treaty) : "Such are a few of the typical 
results of the treaty we helped to make. Some are good, others bad; some spell 
freedom or justice, others spell hatred or revenge, or the greedy grabbing of 
territory that has always led to renewed war." 

Twelve American texts do not even mention the Fourteen Points. Only two 
of them suggest that the treaty violated them. The German books, on the other 
hand, naturally give much space to the treaty, and compare in detail the Four- 
teen Point promises and the provisions of the treaty. 

Children reading and believing Long's text will feel that America was en- 
tirely right in the war, but their attitude towards its fundamental causes and 
toward the treaty will not be so very different from that of liberal Germans. If, 
however, they read the more typical books they will hold Germany entirely 
responsible, and they will either not know that the Fourteen Points existed, or 



SOME RESEARCHES IN RURAL GROUP ANALYSIS 183 

they will feel that the treaty is based upon them. With respect to attitudes 
toward Germany, the choice of texts will be all-important. 

My most general conclusion then is, that unless some other force intervenes, 
the next war is being prepared in the history textbooks of France and Germany, 
and to a lesser degree in those of the United States. 



SOME RESEARCHES IN RURAL GROUP ANALYSIS 



J. H. KOLB, UNIVERSITY OF WISCONSIN 



Group analysis as a method of studying rural society has been, in general, 
of three kinds : the analysis of locality or ecological arrangements, the analysis 
of the interest or intentional arrangements, with particular emphasis upon in- 
stitutional phases, and the analysis of participation and group behavior with 
attention to attitudes. These three phases of study have not been clearly dif- 
ferentiated in the various research projects. Consequently, the classifications 
made here will be open to argument. Time will permit only a few most general- 
ized and dogmatic summaries. Attention will be given largely to results rather 
than to methodology and plans. 

i. The analysis of the locality or ecological arrangements of rural groups 
is represented, among others, by such researches as Galpin's "Trade Area" 
Communities in Wisconsin, Taylor and Zimmermann's Rural Organization in 
North Carolina, Sanderson and Thompson's Social Areas in New York, Baum- 
gartel's Social Study of Ravaille County, Montana, and Kolb's Primary Groups 
in Wisconsin; also by Nelson's Structure of a Village Community in Utah (un- 
published) and B runner's Analysis of One Hundred Fifty American Agricultural 
Villages (unpublished). 

A few results may be summarized by first calling attention to the types of 
locality groups discovered and to their interrelations. First, is the open-country 
neighborhood group which is characterized by primary contacts, simple organ- 
ization, and the discharge of a few services, sometimes by social institutions and 
sometimes by informal arrangements. Second, the country community group. 
This is similar to the first, but larger, somewhat more complex, and more largely 
self-sufficient because of the greater number and diversity of social institutions 
and the larger area. Many of the major interests involving both primary and 
secondary contacts receive attention. Its center is frequently an informal ar- 
rangement of homes and social institutions. It is often called a hamlet. Third, 
is the village or small town group which is characterized by sufficient size and 
complexity to give it a certain amount of self-consciousness and sufficiency. 
These groups may be scaled up or down on the basis of size and type of service 
rendered. Fourth, is the small village neighborhood or primary service area, 
including within its boundaries those farm families who use its name for the 
designation of their own locality and who identify themselves in social and in- 



1 84 



THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 



stitutional arrangements with the center itself. Fifth, is the larger service com- 
munity, which is defined or characterized as "an area tributary to a center of 
common interest." Such an area may easily include one or more open-country 
neighborhoods. Its center is usually a village or small town. The contacts and 
service institutions may be of a primary, secondary, or even specialized, char- 
acter if the center is sufficiently equipped and the area represents a large 
enough economic base. 



12 16 20 24 28 3* 36 40 44 

DISTANCE IN MILES 




A THEORETICAL GRAPH INDICATING THE INTERRELATION OF 

RURAL GROUP AREAS AND TYPES OF SERVICE CENTERS 
Type I, single service (neighborhood or hamlet); type II, limited and simple 
service (small village); type III, semi-complete or intermediate (village or small town); 
Type IV, complete and partially specialized (town or small city) ; type V, urban and 
highly specialized (city). 

P. Primary service area; Sc. Secondary service area; Sp .Specialized service 
area; N. Country neighborhood area. 

No one of these locality groups lives to itself, but each is related to all the 
others. These relationships follow fairly regular forms depending upon the 
types of contacts, the service arrangements, and the " volume of business" as 
represented by extent of area required to make the arrangements effective. The 
accompanying graph is intended to show these interrelationships. 

But changes in this set of locality group arrangements are taking place with 
rapidity and with telling effect. The change of most fundamental importance, 
of course, is the greatly increased mobility of rural people made possible by the 
expanded facilities for communication and transportation. Contacts may now 



SOME RESEARCHES IN RURAL GROUP ANALYSIS 185 

be made on the basis of interest rather than locality. This tends to break down 
many of the older arrangements. Local churches, schools, or clubs are no longer 
strictly local and indigenous. They conform to certain standardized influences 
from "higher up" sources of district conference, county superintendent, or state 
conclave. But as one might expect, the locality groups, which have succeeded 
in institutionalizing one or more of their interests, persist longest. 

2. The analysis of the interest or intentional arrangements of groups, with 
particular emphasis upon institutions, is represented by such researches as 
Mumford's Social Aspects of Consolidated School Areas in Michigan (unpub- 
lished); B runner's Churches of Distinction in Town and Country; Lively 's Rural 
Social Agencies in Ohio; Gooden's Rural Life at its Best in Arkansas; Kolb's Four 
Town-Country Service Institutions (unpublished). 

The interest or intentional rural groups depend more directly upon common 
choices or wishes of their members. These groups may be permanent or tem- 
porary, primary or secondary in their contacts. Locality and interest groups 
frequently oscillate back and forth. Interest groups, for example, may spring 
up within a locality group, producing disorganization and robbing it of its 
geographic significance. On the other hand, an interest group may pass over 
into a group with locality characteristics, as, for example, a religious group by 
some missionary or settlement policy may bring all in a vicinity within its pale. 
The locality groups may be said to have lateral arrangements, and the interest 
groups, perpendicular arrangements. The latter are arranged around a pole or 
axis of common interests largely irrespective of geographic limitations. In 
North Carolina the groups with primary contacts were found to be more defi- 
nitely dependent upon institutional than upon locality arrangements. While in 
Wisconsin, particularly in the rough unglaciated country, locality was the con- 
ditioning factor. In Iowa, because of intentional policies of farm bureau and 
extension service, the township has come to have group significance. In New 
York the primary locality groups have practically lost their significance, being 
replaced by larger community groups in which the social institutions are, to a 
large extent, determining factors. 

3. The analysis of participation and group behavior is represented by such 
researches as Hypes' Social Participation in a Rural New England Town, (un- 
published); Zimmermann's Study of Marketing Attitudes in Minnesota (unpub- 
lished) ; Taylor's Study in the Social Psychology of Farm Organizations (unpub- 
lished); Lindeman's Social Discovery (chapters based on study of marketing 
co-operatives) ; Rankin's Reading in the Farm Home in Nebraska; Kolb's Service 
Relations of Town and Country (one section devoted to measures of farm par- 
ticipation, with reasons for the various kinds of services). The method for 
study in this section has been to analyze forms of organizations which have 
been deliberately projected for some purpose; then a study of customs, tradi- 
tions, and public opinion. The attempt has been to get at the attitudes and 
social values. This has sometimes been attempted through the stimulus-response 
formula. No uniform technique has been developed, in fact, this phase of group 



1 86 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

analysis is still out on the frontier, waiting upon the steadying influences of 
developments in social psychology. 

In the New England study, effort is made to relate participation to structural 
and locality factors, to measure group participation by a family index, and to 
give attention to problems involving social control. The Nebraska study has 
revealed what farm families read. More study needs now to be given to how 
these media controlling communication influence attitudes and group opinion. 
The Wisconsin study of 787 farm families has revealed certain sets of reasons 
for participation in town and country service relationships. Those services in- 
volving primary contacts were dependent upon acquaintanceship, personal 
control, friendship, or kinship. Those involving secondary relationships, such 
as merchandizing, banking, and high school, were dependent upon accessibility. 
When the quality or the price of the service in question was not widely out of 
proportion, participation depended upon "nearness" or "convenience" to the 
center. In the field of specialized services, represented by trade in good clothing, 
hospital or clinic service, and musical productions, variety and selection as well 
as the quality of the service were the reasons given. It is significant to note that 
in participation of leadership as represented by office-holding and committee 
work, the farmer fell far below the ratio of his general activity participation. 
This immediately raises important problems which need further analysis, in- 
volving the question of what is happening when larger impersonal or overhead 
organizations are being slowly substituted for group behavior on the locality 
and primary contact bases. 



COMMODITY DISTRIBUTION IN RURAL COMMUNITIES 



C. R. HOFFER, UNIVERSITY OF MINNESOTA 



This investigation involves a detailed study of 238 stores in twelve rural 
trade centers in Minnesota, though additional data are used to supplement the 
detailed investigation. One purpose of the study was an attempt to construct 
a quantitative expression for service in commodity distribution, service being de- 
fined as the activities and benefits associated with the sale of commodities in 
rural communities. 

In constructing this method of quantitatively expressing service it was 
necessary to select representative items of service, such as retail value of goods 
sold, number of types of stores in a town, advertising quality and price, keeping 
medium to high-grade merchandise, clean walls, etc. Inasmuch as the town was 
considered as a unit, the percentage of stores in it offering these various services 
made a satisfactory basis for recording in quantitative terms the presence of 
these items. For example, if all the stores, or 100 per cent, are clean, the town 
would be given a rating of 100 for this item. If only 50 per cent of the stores 
were clean, then the town would be given 50 points in the rating for this item. 



COMMODITY DISTRIBUTION IN RURAL COMMUNITIES 187 

So in like manner all the other items would be counted. It will be noted that 
this method of quantitatively expressing service does not measure the relative 
importance of the various service items. To do so would involve data and study 
which go beyond the limits of the present investigation. However, a ranking of 
towns based on the relative ratings of service items shows that (i) in general, the 
service of the town increases as it becomes larger, though size of town is not a 
safe criterion of service; (2) the number of stores in a town is also unreliable as 
an index of service; (3) the service which a town gives depends very largely on 
the initiative of the merchants in it. 

Another purpose of this study was to find out what conditions actually 
existed in these 238 representative stores. Only a few points can be noted here. 
It was discovered that the cost of service, that is, what the buyer would have 
to pay the merchant for his services, did not vary appreciably between the large 
and the small town, though the mark-up did vary for different types of stores. 
It was found that efficiency of stores was more important from the standpoint 
of the commnuity than number of stores. It may be stated succinctly, how- 
ever, that stores which handled staple commodities, like drugs, groceries, and 
hardware, tended to increase in number somewhat in a proportional manner to 
the population. Jewelry stores, furniture stores, and ladies-ready-to-wear stores 
did not increase in number so uniformly as the town got larger, but instead the 
store increased in size. Two-thirds of the merchants balanced their books at 
weekly or monthly intervals. The average age of the merchants was forty-four 
years, and the average period of residence in the community where their stores 
were located was twenty-six years. The average period during which the mem- 
bers had been managers of their stores was thirteen years. Eighty-four per cent 
had completed the eighth grade; 39 per cent had attended high school one or 
more years; 7 per cent had attended business college, and 6 per cent had attended 
a college or university. Three-fourths of the merchants had some kind of mer- 
chandising experience before they became managers of their store. Sixty-four 
per cent were members of local business men's organizations of some kind. 

Turning now to a consideration of the comparative amount of service given 
by small and large towns, it was found that there was a minimum-size town for 
stores of different types. A count of the number of different types of stores in 
603 rural towns in Minnesota showed that towns of less than 500 in population 
did not average one drug-store per town. Towns below 1,000 in population did 
not average one furniture store, grocery store, jewelry store, or men's clothing 
store per town. Towns of less than 2,500 in population did not average one shoe- 
store per town. An average of one ladies-ready-to-wear store and one variety 
store were not found in towns having a population of less than 3,000. There was 
no lower limit for hardware and general stores. Moreover, the small town 
lacked certain services of a professional and social sort. Only 94 per cent of the 
towns in Minnesota having a population of approximately 1,250 had a resident 
dentist or dentists, and it was not until towns in die population group of 2,001- 
2,500 were reached that every town had this type of service. Physicians were 



i88 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

more commonly found, but slightly less than half of the towns of less than 1,000 
in population had a physician. Weekly newspapers were published in only 84 
per cent of the fifty- three towns in the 1,001-1,500 population group. However, 
all towns in the 1,501-2,000 group had weekly newspapers. Civic organizations 
of various kinds occurred less frequently in the smaller towns. Slightly over a 
third of the 132 towns in the 501-1,000 group in Minnesota had such an organi- 
zation. Only 10 per cent of these 132 towns had libraries. Such inadequacy of 
services at the trade center makes the trade relations of the farmer disorganized 
and complex, and in a certain measure jeopardizes the possibility of securing 
effective community organization. The farmer is placed in the dilemma of favor- 
ing the small town as a place to market grain, livestock, cream, etc., but when 
the same farmer becomes purchaser and user of goods he is handicapped by the 
small trade center. The way out of the dilemma is to establish, so far as possible, 
consolidated trade-centers towns of approximately 2,500 in population keep 
only those stores in a town which can offer a full quota of service for stores of 
their particular type. Rural schools and churches have been consolidating. The 
time is at hand when trading service must do the same thing. Data have already 
been suggested which show how far this consolidating process may go at the 
present time. Towns of less than 500 in population do not average one drug 
store, hence the logical conclusion to reach is that towns of less than 500 should 
not try to have drug-stores. There is no minimum limit for general stores and 
hardware stores, but for furniture stores, grocery stores, jewelry stores, men's 
clothing stores, the lower limit is a town having a population of not less than 
1,000. For shoe-stores the lower limit is a town of 2,500 in population, and for 
ladies-ready-to-wear and variety stores a town having a population of at least 
3,000. The great waste of small competing trade-centers occurs when they try 
to give a type of service which, when considered from all angles, they cannot 
give so successfully as the larger trade-centers. When small towns work for 
efficiency and quality of service with the stores which they can adequately sup- 
port, rather than to place so much emphasis on types of service which can be 
given more advantageously by larger towns, they will supplement, and not 
duplicate, the service given by the consolidated trade-center. 



AN ESTIMATE OF RURAL MIGRATION AND OTHER 
SOURCES OF URBAN INCREASE 



JOHN M. GILLETTE, UNIVERSITY or NORTH DAKOTA 



Perhaps the most satisfactory way to treat this subject is to exhibit first the 
results obtained in tabular form and then offer a few remarks concerning the 
methods used. 

TABLE I 

SOURCES OF URBAN INCREASE FOR DECADES ENDING 1910 AND 1920 



SOURCE 


NUMBER CONTRIBUTED 


PERCENTAGE CONTRIBUTED 


IQIO 


1920 


IQIO 


1920 


Rural migration . . 


3,637,000 
2,426,000 
4,849,000 
924,000 


5,476,000 
2,842,000 

2 , 830 , OOO 
99O,OOO 


30.7 
20.5 
41.0 
7-8 


45-2 
23-4 
23-3 
8.1 


Natural increase 


Immigration 


Incorporation . .... 


Total 


11,826,000 


I2,I38,OOO 


100. 


IOO.O 





TABLE II 
SOURCES OF RURAL INCREASE AND DECREASE FOR THE DECADE ENDING 1920 



Source 


Contributed 


Lost 


Natural increase 


7,087,000 




Immigration 


7 tjj. OOO 




Incorporation 




OOO . OOO 


Rural migration 




e. c84.ooo 


Census increase (difference) . 




i 600 ooo 









According to Table I, the amount of rural migration is about 5,500,000 for 
the last decade, as compared to about 3,500,000 during the preceding decade. 
If we add the amount of incorporation in each case, we find that practically 
6,500,000 persons from territory that was rural in 1910 shifted to territory urban 
in 1920. This represents the population of a city as large as New York, that of 
more than two cities the size of Chicago, and that of more then ten cities the 
size of Boston or San Francisco. In other words, the annual drain of population 
from the country is more than equal to that required to build a Boston or a San 
Francisco each year of the decade. 

We note that for the last decade, rural migration is the leading source of 

189 



190 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

urban increase, being 45 per cent of such increase. Rural migration and incor- 
poration together make up about 54 per cent of urban increase. In the decade 
ending 1910, immigration stood first as a cause of urban growth, then accounting 
for 41 per cent of that growth. Rural migration then stood second in importance. 

The country side of the ledger presents a different page. The sources of 
increase in the country are only two as against four for the city. Natural in- 
crease contributes 7,087,000 and immigration 754,000. The country suffers great 
losses : 990,000 by incorporation and 5, 584,000 by migration to cities. The census 
credits a gain of only 1,600,000 to rural districts for the period. 

A few remarks concerning the methods used in making the estimates are in 
order. 

As a means of getting a perspective and basis for checking other operations, 
a national rate of natural increase was first estimated. To do this, an estimate 
of the total force of immigration of aliens and the emigration of citizens was 
required. In treating immigration, the decennial balance between alien income 
and outgo, as given in the reports of the commissioner of immigration, was 
adjusted to census dates. The immigrant birth-rate is the weighted average 
of the foreign rates of the twelve leading stocks and their proportion in total 
immigration. The resulting rate was adjusted to proportion of sexes and age 
distribution obtaining among immigrants and was found to be 24.7, somewhat 
smaller than that for the nation. The application of our national rate would 
have produced only slight variations in the final results. 

The death-rate used for immigrants was that of the nation, adjusted to 
their age distribution. So adjusted, it is 9.8, possibly too low a rate. I was 
tempted to use the weighted rate estimated from death-rates and numbers of 
decedents of foreign-born mothers given in the United States mortality rates 
for 1920, but that procedure seemed unjustified. 

Certain other adjustments of a minor nature were made during the course 
of estimating the birth- and death-rates. The resulting rate of natural increase 
for immigrants was 14.9, about 27 per cent higher than that for the nation. 

The rate of natural increase of citizen emigrants for the decade was also 
obtained. The total force of emigration was added to the increase of the national 
population for the decade ending 1920. From this was taken the total force of 
immigration. The difference represents the natural increase in the population 
of 1910, the rate of increase being 11.7. This is two points below my estimated 
rate for 1900-1910. It agrees with the weighted rate obtained by treating rural 
and urban populations. 

In estimating the rates of natural increase for rural and urban districts, the 
following steps were taken. First, establishing birth-rates. To the number of 
infants under one year of age in 1920 was added the number of infant decedents 
during the previous year. The result was divided by the estimated population 
living at the beginning of the year. Then the average of this and my estimated 
rate for the previous decade was taken. This process was applied to both rural 
and urban populations, the resulting birth-rates being: rural, 28.2; urban, 23.2. 



ASPECTS AND TENDENCIES OF THE RACE PROBLEM 191 

I employed this method rather than using the census birth-rates because by 
it I secured rates for the total national population. I employed a check on these 
rates by comparing their ratio to each other with that of rural to urban children 
under five per 1,000 rural and urban females of the age period 15-44. My urban 
birth-rate is 82 per cent of the rural rate, but the proportion of urban children 
per 1,000 females is only 73 per cent of that of rural districts. Consequently the 
differences between my rural and urban birth-rates must be regarded as very 
conservative. 

Second, ascertaining death-rates. The death-rates for rural and urban popu- 
lations given in the census volume, Mortality Rates, for each year were averaged 
for the decade in question. These were for the registration area only, instead of 
for the total population, as was the case with the birth-rates. The consequent 
rates of natural increase were: rural, 15.2, urban, 7.6. As previously remarked, 
when weighted by the rural and urban populations and averaged, the resulting 
national rate agrees with that obtained by another method. 

From data in the census, it is learned what proportion of net immigration 
of the last decade lived in city and country. Then to obtain the contributions 
immigration made to city and country growth it is only necessary to apportion 
the total immigration previously estimated between rural and urban districts, 
the amounts being: rural, 754,000; urban, 2,830,000. 

Incorporation represents the population living in that portion of urban 
territory in 1920 that was rural in 1910, together with its natural increase and 
less that portion of immigration resident there but already accounted for under 
immigration. Incorporation so computed amounted to 990,000. 

I have been compelled to identify farm population with rural population in 
making these estimates, because no data exist for the former as a separate 
entity. But it is likely that the farm population constitutes about 60 or 65 per 
cent of what the census denominates "rural population." We might think that 
some such percentage of the rural migration emanates from the open country. 



ASPECTS AND TENDENCIES OF THE RACE PROBLEM 

(1912-24) 



MONROE N. WORK 
Tuskegee Normal and Industrial Institute 



In what follows there is given the more important results of a study of 
aspects and tendencies of the race problem in the past thirteen years. I present 
first what I call, for the want of a better term, some secondary aspects of the 
problem. 

i. There is the economic progress of the group. In the past ten years 
Negroes have entered industry in a large way. The 1920 Census reports 332,249 



192 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

Negroes engaged in skilled and semi-skilled work. The wealth of the group in 
1912 was estimated to be $700,000,000. The wealth of the group at present is 
around $2,000,000,000, which is one and one-half times more wealth than it 
had accumulated up to 1912. 

2. There is the educational progress of the group. The total amount ex- 
pended in 1912 for all phases of Negro education was $13,576,561; the amount 
expended this year for Negro education is over $40,000,000. The past thirteen 
years have witnessed an ever increasing demand in all lines of work for the 
educated Negro. The improvement in education is reflected in the increase in 
the number of students in elementary, secondary, and higher courses. This im- 
provement is especially reflected in the increase in the number completing col- 
lege courses. Up to 1912 about 5,000 Negroes had completed college courses. 
In the period 1912-24 about 5,000 Negroes graduated from college. That is, in 
the past thirteen years as many Negroes graduated from college as in all the 
previous years. 

3. There is the progress which, in the past thirteen years, has been made in 
health improvement. The Negro now has a declining mortality rate and an 
increasing life-span. In 1912 the death-rate per thousand was 22.9. In 1922, ten 
years later, the death-rate was 15.7 per thousand; a decrease for the period of 
31.5 per cent. A recent study of mortality among the 1,800,000 Negro policy- 
holders of the Metropolitan Life Insurance Company indicates that since 1912 
there has been an increase of five years in the life-expectancy of the Negro. In 
1912 the average expectancy by life for the Negro Metropolitan policyholders, 
male and female, of all ages from two years up, was: for males 41.32 years; for 
females, 41.30 years. In 1922 the expectancy of life was for males 46.91 years; 
for females, 46.10 years. In 1912 the life-span for the Negroes of the country as 
a whole was about 35 years. It is now about 40 years. That is, since 1912 the 
life-span of the Negroes of the country has been increased five years. 

4. There is the Negro in politics. The period under consideration witnessed 
an increased activity of the Negro in politics. Some striking features of this 
activity were: The tendency to develop independence in politics; an increasing 
number of Negroes voting the Democratic ticket. Political parties are making 
efforts to get the Negro vote. In the presidential campaign which has just 
closed, special Negro campaign bureaus were maintained by the Republican, 
Democratic, and Third Party national campaign committees. There is an in- 
crease in the number of Negroes elected to office. In 1914 there were eight Negro 
members of city councils. In 1921, these members numbered twenty-one. There 
were, in 1915, two Negro members of state legislatures; in 1924 there were 
twelve. It is also of importance to note that, in 1915, 1 had a record of fourteen 
cities with Negro policemen; in 1924 there are seventy cities which have Negro 
policemen. The entry and activity of Negro women in politics also is notable, 
as well as the gradual increase in the South of the number of Negroes qualified 
to vote, and the increased efforts of Negroes to vote in and break down the 
"white primary" in the South. With the Negro dividing his vote, and an increas- 



ASPECTS AND TENDENCIES OF THE RACE PROBLEM 193 

ing number voting the Democratic ticket, the maintenance of the so-called 
"white primary" becomes a more difficult problem. 1 

5. There are the population shifts which have taken place in the past thir- 
teen years. During this period there occurred the greatest movement of Ne- 
groes which has ever taken place in this country. In addition to the migra- 
tion from the country to the cities and from the South to the North, 200,000 
Negro soldiers were transported to Europe and back again. The net result of 
the migration is that there are now almost a million more Negroes living in 
cities than there were thirteen years ago, and over half a million more Negroes 
living in the North than were in 1912. 

We pass to the consideration of some primary aspects of the race problem. 
One of these is lynching. Beginning with 1912, there has been increased public- 
ity in the press of the country as a whole with reference to lynching. There has 
been in recent years a striking growth of public opinion against lynching. In the 
period, 1912-24, eight states passed laws designed to check lynching. A federal 
bill against lynching is now pending in Congress. There has been in the past 
thirteen years a notable decrease in the number of lynchings. In the period 
1912-24 there were 705 lynchings. This was 40 per cent less than the number, 
1,177, for the previous thirteen years, and 67 per cent less than the number, 
2,137, f r the thirteen years before that time. 

The second primary aspect of the problem is segregation, (i) The efforts 
to restrict by law the areas in which Negroes shall live began in 1911. From 
then to 1917 a number of cities in border and southern states passed segrega- 
tion laws. (2) In 1917 the United States Supreme Court declared the segrega- 
tion laws invalid. (3) Since 1917, two new devices have become evolved for 
legalizing segregation: (a) By zoning ordinances, (b) By property-owners' con- 
tract. (4) The legality of these methods is now being tested in the courts. (5) 
In numerous instances bombing and other violent methods have been used in 
attempting to intimidate and drive out Negroes. 

The third primary aspect of the problem is riots. The thirteen years, 1912- 
24, have been notable for the number of race riots which have occurred. Some of 
these riots were the most serious which have taken place in the history of the 
nation. Some of the immediate causes of the riots were: the migration of 

1 In 1922 the supreme court of the state of Texas ruled that the Democratic party 
had a right to hold a "white primary." In 1923 the Texas legislature passed a law pro- 
hibiting Negroes from participating in "Democratic primaries." October 20, 1924, the 
United States Supreme Court ruled out, on the ground that "cause of action had ceased 
to exist," a case brought in 1921 by Negroes of Houston, Texas, to restrain the election 
judges of that city from holding a strictly "white voters' primary." On July 26, 1924, 
Dr. L. A. Nixon, colored, of El Paso, Texas, and a regular Democrat of many years' 
standing who had voted in previous Democratic primaries, was denied the privilege of 
casting his ballot. He brought suit for $5,000 damages against the election judges and 
to test the constitutionality of the law. This case is being carried through the Texas 
courts up to the United States Supreme Court. 



194 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

Negroes; an intensified housing situation; exaggerated reports concerning crimes 
by Negroes; the state of the public mind with reference to the Negro and his 
place; the disposition of Negroes not to recede from what they considered a just 
position. Another probable cause was that, almost without exception, the wide- 
spread presentation of the Negro in moving pictures was either as a buffoon or 
a criminal. 

The fourth aspect of the problem is the notable growth of race conscious- 
ness which has taken place since 1912. Chief among the causes of this growth 
are: (i) The general rapid economic and educational progress of the group. 
(2) The world- war conditions. (3) The contacts which Negroes throughout the 
world have established in recent years with each other. Manifestations of this 
race consciousness are a growing race pride, increasing race solidarity, the de- 
velopment of race literature, including hymns and poems which are national 
in their expressions, and an increasing effort of the group to gain for itself those 
rights and privileges which are its due. 

The fifth primary aspect of the problem is the increase in efforts for the 
betterment of race relations. There were, first, joint conferences of whites and 
Negroes on race relations, where face to face they talked to, and not about, each 
other. A second factor has been the presentation of facts rather than expres- 
sions of opinion. Among the chief ageneies for the general dissemination of these 
facts were the Negro Year Book, first published in 1912, the Southern Publicity 
Committee, and the Hampton Institute Press Service. A third factor is the 
growth of inter-racial co-operation. Whites and Negroes worked together in 
the world-war period. The result of their working together in the world-war 
activities brought them into a more helpful relationship. Out of the spirit of 
the war co-operation, the after-war inter-racial co-operation developed. In 
November, 1918, the Commission on Inter-racial Co-operation was formed and 
an organized effort was begun to better race relations. The Commission has 
organized state inter-racial committees of whites and Negroes in most of the 
southern states, and local inter-racial committees in some eight hundred coun- 
ties in the south. 

A fourth important factor has been the changed attitude and the activities 
of the church with respect to race relation. Some two years ago, the Federal 
Council of Churches established a commission on race relations which is carry- 
ing on an active campaign to assist in improving race relations. An important 
feature of this activity is the annual observance of race-relations Sunday, at 
which time an effort is made to have sermons on race relations preached in pul- 
pits throughout the nation. A fifth important feature is the increasing tendency 
of white women's organizations in the South to work for the betterment of 
Negroes and to bring about co-operation of white and Negro women. A sixth 
feature is the striking growth of efforts to study the problem. There are the 
Phelps-Stokes Fund fellowships for the study of the Negro at the universities 
of Virginia and Georgia; The Y.M.C.A. study course on the Negro in white 
colleges of the South; the establishing, in 1915, of the Association for the Study 



ASPECTS AND TENDENCIES OF THE RACE PROBLEM 195 

of Negro Life and History; the increasing number of courses on the Negro and 
on race relations in universities and colleges. In 1922 the study of the Negro 
was made the home-missions study subject for mission classes in the churches 
of the country. This greatly stimulated the writing of books on the study of 
the Negro, with the result that in the three years, 1922-24, there have been 
twenty- two race-relations study books published, five of which are for juveniles, 
and seventeen for adults. 1 

1 Race relations study books: (i) For juveniles: The Magic Box, Anita B. Ferris, 
Council of Women for Home Missions and Missionary Education Movement of the 
United States and Canada, New York, 1922; The Handicapped Winners: a Race-Rela- 
tions Reader, Sara Estelle Haskins, Board of Missions (Women's Work) of the Methodist 
Episcopal Church, South, Nashville (Tennessee), 1922; The Stories Of Black FolkFor 
Little Folk, Bessie Landrum, the A. B. Caldwell Publishing Company, Atlanta, 1923; 
A Boy's Life of Booker T. Washington, W. C. Jackson, the Macmillan Company, New 
York, 1922; The Negro Boy and Girl: Study Book for Juniors, S. J. Fisher, Board of 
. Missions for Freedmen of the Presbyterian Church in the United States of America, 
Pittsburgh, 1923. (2) For adults: The Trend of the Races: a Home-Mission Study Book, 
George E. Haynes, Council of Women for Home Missions and Missionary Education 
Movement of the United States and Canada, New York, 1922; Race Grit; Adventures 
on the Borderland of Liberty (for mission-study classes and also for the general reader) , 
Coe Hayne, Department of Missionary Education, Board of Education of the Northern 
Baptist Convention, the Judson Press, Philadelphia, 1922; Wanted Leaders: a Study 
of Negro Development, Theodore D. Bratton, Department of Missions and Church 
Extension, the Episcopal Church, New York, 1922; Wanted Leaders: a Study of Negro 
Development; Suggestions for Group Discussion and Individual Study (guide for the use 
of Bishop B ration's book), Laura F. Boyer, Department of Missions and Church Ex- 
tension, New York, 1922; In the Vanguard of a Race, L. H. Hammond, Council of 
Women for Home Missions and Missionary Education Movement of the United States 
and Canada, New York, 1922; The Negro from Africa to America, W. D. Weatherford, 
George H. Doran Company, New York, 1924; The Clash of Color, Basil Mathews, 
Missionary Education Movement of the United States and Canada, New York, 1924; 
Of One Blood: a Short Study of the Race Problem, Robert E. Speer, Council of Women 
for Home Missions and Missionary Education Movement of the United States and 
Canada, New York, 1924; Race And Race Relations: a Christian View of Human Con- 
tacts, Robert E. Speer, Fleming H. Revell Company, New York, 1924; Christianity and 
the Race Problem, J. H. Oldham, Student Christian Movement, London, 1924. The 
Problem of Race (a study outline based on Oldham's Christianity and the Race Problem), 
Christian Student Movement, London, 1924; The Gift of Black Folk: the Negroes in the 
Making of America, W. E. B. DuBois, "Knights of Columbus Contribution Series," the 
Stratford Company, Boston, 1924; And Who Is My Neighbor? An Outline Study of Race 
Relations in America, Part I, Association Press, New York, 1924; Racial Relations and 
the Christian Ideal: a Discussion Course for College Students, Student Volunteer Move- 
ment, New York, 1923; The Basis of Racial Adjustment: a Study Outline and Readings 
On the Progress of the Negro, T. J. Woofter, Jr., Ginn and Company, New York, 1925; 
Race Prejudice, Erie Fiske Young, Chicago, 1925; Races, Nations and Classes, Herbert 
Adolphus Miller (for advanced study of race conflict), J. B. Lippincott Company, 
Philadelphia, 1924. 



196 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

The tendencies of the race problem in the United States since 1912 appears 
to be as follows: 

1. There is a tendency for the more acute and constant points of race fric- 
tion to shift from rural districts to urban centers. 

2. The general advance of the Negroes has tended (a) to change their 
status, (b) to change their outlook, and (c) to bring them into contact with the 
white group in a greater and more varied number of ways. 

3. These contacts with the white group tend more and more to be contacts 
with the progressive, the intelligent elements of the Negro group. 

4. These newer and more numerous contacts in some instances tend to 
create conflict situations which turn primarily not upon the ignorance, the 
backwardness of the Negro group, but upon its progressiveness, its intelligence. 
The agitation and friction now going on as to where Negroes shall live in cities 
centers about the efforts of progressive intelligent members of the group to 
secure better places in which to live. 

5. There is a growing tendency, particularly in the South, to endeavor to 
handle the problems of race relations by whites and Negroes coming together on 
a basis of co-operation and working together for the best interests of both races. 
The general advantage of this method is that representatives of both groups 
may meet face to face and outline policies which are of mutual benefit to each 
of the groups and to the whole community. 



DENSITY OF POPULATION AND THE STANDARD OF 
LIVING IN NORTH CHINA 



C. G. DlTTMER 



During the years 1914 to 1917, and 1918 to 1921, while teaching in the 
American Indemnity College in Peking, China, I interested myself in the study 
of population pressure in North China and its effect on the standard of living. 
The study was made more difficult, but also more interesting, due to the fact 
that nothing had as yet been done on the standard of living in China, that there 
were no vital statistics of any value whatever, and that even the official census 
reports represented little more than "pagodas of guesses." 

We know that the area of China Proper (the eighteen provinces) is ap- 
proximately one-half that of the United States of America, and that the various 
official and semi-official estimates of the population vary all the way from 
350,000,000 to 450,000,000. The 1902 Census, which is as valuable as any, 
gives the population of China Proper as 410,000,000, and places the crude 
density of the whole at 268 per English square mile, with provincial densities 
ranging all the way from 66 for Kwangsi to 683 for Shantung. 

Crude densities of this sort are of little value unless we know something of 
the stage of economic development of the country and its geographical environ- 



POPULATION AND LIVING IN NORTH CHINA 197 

ment. It is impossible to compare American and European densities with those 
of China, due to the fact that China is still supported almost entirely by a 
medieval type of agriculture and without the extensive aid of supporting in- 
dustries. She has not even sufficiently developed means of communication to 
make effective trade and commerce possible between districts one hundred 
miles apart. 

China's population, aside from a small number of large cities, is one of agri- 
cultural villages ranging in size from 25 to 500 families. One hundred families 
seems to be the most usual size, and these are distributed at the rate of about 
one to the square mile in arable sections. 

My first problem was to gain some idea of the effective density of population. 
This was done by the sampling process, and was facilitated by the fact that the 
entire agricultural population lives in village rather than in separate farm-home 
units. The following results were obtained. 

The county of Tang in the province of Chihli, according to figures furnished 
by the chief magistrate of the county, has an area of 900 square miles and a 
crude density of 189. This seemed impossible, but then, his figures also showed 
the females of the county to represent but 39 per cent of the total population. 
It is not strange that we thought it worth while to gather some statistics of our 
own, and in so doing marked off an area of 20 square miles and found it to 
include 50 villages ranging in size from 20 to 500 families and with an effective 
density of 2,600 to the arable square mile. 

In Wang Tu and adjoining county we found but 956 to the square mile, 
and this county was more fertile, more prosperous, and nearer the railroad. 

In an industrial district where agriculture is supplemented by handicraft 
weaving we found a density of 2,000. 

In Shantung, with a crude density of 683 to the square mile we found 
densities of 2,500, 2,700, and 3,000. 

In Jao Yang County, in an area of 200 square miles there are 214 villages 
and a density of population of 1,500. 

In the metropolitan district to the west of Peking, destitute Manchus, who 
prefer genteel starvation to manual labor, are existing at the rate of 2,000 to the 
square mile. 

Professor Tayler, in his post-famine studies, presents thirteen cases which 
range from 290 to 6,880 to the square mile. 

A group of Congregational missionaries located in the city of Paotingfu 
have, with no attempt at scientific accuracy, conservatively estimated the crude 
density of the 25 counties which comprise their field at 1,000. As this area is 
fairly mountainous, the effective density is probably twice that amount. 

As a result of these samples and other available estimates I have come to 
the conclusion that we may expect in agricultural China, and that is practically 
all of China, a density, varying with the fertility of the soil and other determin- 
ing factors of productivity, of anything between 1,500 and 2,500 to the square 
mile. We would be quite safe in striking an average of 2,000, but even the lowest 



198 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

figure is bad enough. A condition of this sort means farms of between one and 
two acres per family, and that farm villages are as close together as farmhouses 
are here in the Middle West. The post-famine studies of Professor Tayler show 
that one-third of the farms are less than one acre in size, two-thirds are less than 
two acres, and that only one-tenth of i per cent are as large as 160 acres, a very 
common size in America. 

My second problem was that of the standard of living which might be 
maintained under conditions of such density. My first findings, based on the 
study of 195 families in a rural district near Peking, were published in the 
Quarterly Journal of Economics of November, 1918. During the next three years 
I studied 434 more families in six different provinces. My study followed the 
same general lines as that of Engle in Saxony and the various similar studies 
which have been made in the United States. As house and land are usually 
owned by the peasants of North China I included the rent value of these items 
as a part of their income. Due to the fact that much of the fuel used is gathered 
in the form of twigs and stubble from the field and mountain side, I estimated 
the value of such fuel gathered and included it as a part of the income. As it 
will be impossible to present the entire content of my tables I will content my- 
self with giving an idea of the extremes of variation and a description of the 
standard of living of the modal family. Please bear in mind that all sums of 
money mentioned are in terms of Chinese currency, commonly called "Mex," 
and are the equivalent of approximately one-half that amount in our money. 

Families were classified in $25 income groups, and incomes were found to 
range from $20 to $1,000 per year. The modal group has an income of $82 per 
year, but saving does not seem to begin to take place till incomes of $100 or over 
have been reached. 

In the lowest expenditure group, 71 per cent of the income goes for food 
alone, i per cent for clothing, 24.5 per cent for fuel, 2.5 per cent for house rent, 
nothing for land, and i per cent for all other miscellaneous purposes. These are 
on the basis of incomes of $20 per year, and include a deficit of $0.50. These 
families live in one-room houses, have no land of their own, and gather all the 
fuel they use from the fields. 

In contrast to this the $1,000 families at the upper end of the scale spend 
46 per cent on food, 12 per cent on clothing, 5 per cent on light and fuel, 3 per 
cent on house, 5 per cent on land, and have 29 per cent remaining for various 
miscellaneous purposes. This family arrives at the end of the year with a surplus 
of $200, lives in a ten-room house, buys all the fuel it consumes, and has fifteen 
acres of land. 

These are the extremes, and of them we may conclude that the first would 
not sustain life in America, and that the highest is below what we would consider 
a normal minimum. 

Let us turn to the modal group and see what the standard is. They have, as 
we have already pointed out, a yearly income of $82. They arrive at the end of 



POPULATION AND LIVING IN NORTH CHINA 199 

the year with a deficit of $1.26. This family is composed of 4.4 individuals of 
whom 1.6 are children. There have been numerous other children, but most of 
them have died or disappeared in early infancy. They live in a house of 4.8 
rooms which, if other things were equal, would indicate no evidence of crowding. 
The rent value of the house is $4.15 per year, and that is all it would be worth 
in any place. It is a pretty poor sort of a hovel, built about one end and part of 
the side of a mud-walled courtyard. They have two-thirds of an English acre 
of land from which they gain practically all of their support. For food they 
spend $55.13, which is 67 per cent of their income. They eat meat but once a 
year, and live on two meals per day. Meat and tea are the only luxuries they have 
ever tasted, and it is a fact that they have never had what we would call a 
square meal in their lives. Their clothing costs $3.09, or some 3.7 per cent of 
their income. This amount is hardly sufficient to maintain their clothing in 
proper repair, to say nothing of acquiring new garments. The value of the fuel 
they consume is $9.82, but more than half of the amount is gathered from the 
fields, and the main money expenditure is for oil for lighting. It requires a full 
day's work to provide a day's fuel for the family. Thus nearly 1 2 per cent goes 
for this purpose. For miscellaneous purposes the family spends the magnificent 
amount of $3.89. This amounts to 4.7 per cent of their income and has to supply 
everything aside from the barest necessities of life. 

A standard of living like this is the price the inhabitants of North China 
must pay for one of the most serious conditions of overpopulation on record. It 
is a bare subsistence standard which is maintained in the face of a tremendous 
birth-rate only because the death-rate is equally high. There is no evidence that 
the population of China is increasing at all, and there is every evidence that the 
standard of living has struck bottom; that a Malthusian balance has been at 
last attained. 

From these and other studies I have concluded that $100 is necessary to 
keep a Chinese family in normal comfort according to local standards. Approxi- 
mately one-half of my families live on less than this amount. 

On the basis of a normal efficiency diet prepared at the Peking Union Medical 
College for the North China family of five, it has been estimated 1 that the cost 
of food alone should be between $150 and $160 when we consider that 71 per 
cent of our families have less than this amount for all purposes, we have a strik- 
ing indication of the problem which China is facing. 

The Emperor Chien Lung anticipated Malthus by five years when he called 
upon all classes of his subjects to "economize the gifts of heaven, lest, ere long, 
the people exceed the means of subsistence." But the voice they have heeded 
has been that of the sage which warned them that "of the three great evils, the 
greatest is to die without posterity." Young China is now preaching a new gos- 
pel to the effect that "of the three great evils, the greatest is to bring more 
children into the world than can be properly supported." 

1 By Professor J. B. Tayler. 



200 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

A DEPENDENCY INDEX FOR MINNEAPOLIS 



F. STUART CHAPIN, UNIVERSITY OF MINNESOTA 



The quantitative study of social change is a subject of growing interest 
among sociologists, and a matter of theoretical significance as well as of prac- 
tical importance. The rise and fall of dependency with waves of depression and 
prosperity, with the annual change from winter to summer conditions, and as 
the aftermath of wars and famines are phenomena familiar to all history. And 
yet the possibility of accurate quantitative description of these phenomena has 
seldom been seriously considered. 

The present paper is a preliminary report of an effort to describe changes in 
the amount of dependency over a period of nine years and nine months begin- 
ning January, 1915, for the city of Minneapolis. If we are successful in accurate 
quantitative description of the past and present of the phenomenon, we have 
taken the first step toward scientific prediction of its future course, wherein 
lies the practical significance of our study. 

PREPARATION OF THE DATA 

This study was begun by securing the figures for total case load from the 
Family Welfare Association, the Public Poor Relief Department, the University 
Free Dispensary, and the total admissions to the Municipal Free Lodging House 
of Minneapolis, for the period from January, 1915, to September, 1924, inclusive 
by months. These gross totals were then in each case converted into a series of 
relative index numbers with the average monthly total of the year 1919 as a 
base. 1 The series fall into two different types: one which fluctuates violently 
from high peaks to low troughs (Family Welfare, Poor Relief, and Municipal) ; 
and one which shows but minor variations from month to month or year to year. 

ANALYSIS OF THE DATA 

These four series of index numbers exhibit four different kinds of change 
which are particularly evident in the relief series: (i) A long-term upward or 
downward tendency, technically known as the secular trend; (2) a wavelike 
movement of two or more years, known as a cycle; (3) an annual change from 
winter to summer, known as seasonal variation; (4) certain irregular fluctua- 
tions. Since these four kinds of change are all superimposed upon one another, 

1 This study was conducted under the direction of Dr. F. Stuart Chapin, chairman 
of the Department of Sociology, University of Minnesota, in a graduate seminar in 
Statistical Theory in Relation to Social Theory and Practice, and the statistical work 
was done by the following graduate students: Mr. John F. Markey, Mr. Henry C. 
Mohler, Mrs. Helen Kittridge, Miss Jessie Ravitch, and Miss Dorothy P. Gary. 

The year 1919 was selected as the base year because in many respects it showed 
fewer irregular and violent fluctuations in relief, and because hi this year the disturbing 
effects of the influenza epidemic of 1918 were absent, thus making the medical-relief 
series more normal. 



A DEPENDENCY INDEX FOR MINNEAPOLIS 201 

it becomes necessary to separate from the total complex one factor at a time if 
we are to succeed in scientific analysis and description of our problem. 

In attacking this problem we have utilized the technique of statistical- 
mathematical analysis developed and applied by students of the business cycle. 
In two of our series (Family Welfare Association and University Dispensary) 
we have assumed a straight-line trend. In two of our series (Public Poor Relief 
and Municipal Lodging) we were obliged to assume a parabolic movement. 1 
Having measured the secular trend, it becomes necessary to eliminate it from 
the complex and to measure the cycle. But since the cyclical movement is fur- 
ther complicated by an overlay of seasonal variation and irregular fluctuations, 
we have measured these and eliminated them from the cycles. 2 We have now 
completed the preliminary quantitative description of our variables. It re- 
mains to consider the practical import of these findings. Since one test of sci- 
ence is its power to predict future events, we may ask whether we can forecast 
with any assurance the course of dependency in Minneapolis over the year be- 
ginning October, 1924. The degree with which we can satisfy this test is a 
measure of the immediate practical importance of our results. 

PRACTICAL APPLICATIONS 

Minute analysis of the cycles of the Family Welfare index 3 shows that 
the average period from crest to crest is 10.2 months, and from trough to 
trough is 10.5 months, but this movement is further complicated by minor 
cycles in which the average period from crest to crest is 3.76 months, and 

1 The parabolic equations are as follows: Trend of Public Poor Relief series, Trend 
of Municipal Lodging House series, y 364.2 g^x+^Sx*. 

3 Persons link-relative method was not used in this study in the elimination of the 
seasonal factor and of irregular fluctuations, since a shorter and less cumbersome 
method, namely that of Falkner's ratio- to-ordinate method (Journal American Sta- 
tistical Association, Vol. XIX, No. 146) was available, which seemed as effective as 
Persons method in the measurement of seasonal and irregular fluctuations. Specifi- 
cally, the following methods were used for the series indicated (secular trend having 
been eliminated in the first place in those cases where Falkner's method was used) : 
Family Welfare series secular trend linear Davies' method of monthly means for 

measurement of seasonal (Davies, Introd. Economic Statistics, chap, v, pp. 100- 

130) 

University Dispensary series secular trend linear Davies' method of monthly means 
Public Poor Relief series parabolic trend Falkner's ratio-to-ordinate method for 

measurement of seasonal and irregular fluctuations 
Municipal Lodging House series parabolic trend, ;y= 364. 2 9.2*-}- .078:*? Falkner's 

ratio-to-ordinate method 

a The Family Welfare series is the only one of our series which has been studied to 
date in connection with Snyder's index. Hence only the results of this comparison are 
here given. We are now studying every one of our five series (including a series based 
on figures from the Visiting Nurse Association of Minneapolis) in connection with 
Snyder's series as well as several local economic series. 



202 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

from trough to trough, is 4 months. Clearly it is difficult to tell at the beginning 
of a movement whether it is going to be a four-month or a ten-month cycle. 

If we could now discover some other series in which the cyclical movement 
preceded, but was associated with, our dependency cycle, we should then be in 
position to anticipate future movements with an exactitude that would depend 
on the regularity of the lag between the two series. After some experimentation 
we have found such a series in Carl Snyder's new Clearings Index of Business. 1 
To forestall misunderstanding, it should be stated at this point that although we 
are interested in cause-and-effect relations, we make no assumptions or infer- 
ences in this respect regarding the problem under discussion, since this is a 
matter that lies beyond the scope of the data available to the present brief 
inquiry. 

We find in brief: (i) That there is a correlation of .556 between the Family 
Welfare series and Snyder's index for a six-month lag. Further analysis shows 
(2) that the average period between the trough of Snyder's series and the next 
crest of dependency, or between a crest of Snyder's index and the next trough 
of dependency, is 5.60 months for twenty-two different periods. (3) The cor- 
relation coefficient of these twenty-two selected high-low, or low-high points in 
association is .6894. Finally, (4) study of periods of major rise and major 
decline in the two series shows that the Family Welfare Association index tends 
to rise or fall for a period longer by one or two months than the preceding fall or 
rise in Snyder's index. 

SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS 

We may in conclusion venture to predict that (i) if the upward rise of Snyder's 
index since September, 1923, is part of an eight-month rise, then there should be 
an end of the present rise of the dependency index about December, 1924; (2) if 
the upward rise of Snyder's index since September, 1923, is part of a fourteen- 
month rise, then we may not expect a marked turn in the high tide of dependency 
till May or June, 1925. (3) The correlation coefficient for six-month lags suggest 
an earlier fall in the dependency index than either of the two preceding fore- 
casts. These predictions are not certainties; they are rather probabilities. Fur- 
ther analysis of the data will, we fully believe, lead to the discovery of more 
exact and reliable methods of prediction. 

1 See Journal Amer. Statistical Assoc., Vol. XIX, No. 147, pp. 329 ff. 



REPORT OF THE COMMITTEE ON SOCIAL RESEARCH, 
OF THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY, 1924 

The work of the Committee consisted mainly of a survey to determine the 
nature and extent of research being done by the members of the Society. 
Letters were sent out to all members asking them to send to the Committee a 
brief statement regarding any research upon which they were engaged. Replies 
to these letters revealed 204 different reaserch projects being conducted by 

TYPES OF SOCIAL RESEARCH 

Rural sociology 33 

Social psychology 18 

Standard of living 16 

Community life 16 

Dependency 1 1 

Educational sociology 10 

Race 9 

Miscellaneous and studies of social work 9 

Population and migration 9 

Marriage and the family 7 

Social medical 7 

Social change 6 

Religion 6 

Juvenile delinquency 6 

Penology 6 

Labor and industry 6 

Social economics 5 

Social politics 4 

Mental hygiene 4 

Law and sociology 3 

Social control 3 

Personality studies 3 

Sociological theories 3 

Heredity 3 

Methods i 

Ethics i 

Social origins i 

members of the society; some members being engaged, of course, upon more than 
one piece of research. 

From the account of these researches certain classifications were made. 
About one- third (35 per cent) of these reports were on local studies, dealing 
distinctly with a definite and limited area. Thirty-five per cent also were of the 
survey type; that is, they were investigations or surveys of a locality or of a 

203 



204 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

community situation. In 36 per cent of these reports a definite statistical meth- 
odology was used, sufficient to warrant calling them statistical studies. There 
were a number of these researches which were not described fully enough to 
make it possible to say definitely whether they were local studies, of the survey 
type, or statistical in character. Between 30 and 40 per cent were thus incom- 
pletely described. 

We have further attempted to indicate the nature of these pieces of research 
by the assortment shown on page 203. 

The basis of this assortment is what appeared to the Committee to be the 
major idea of the work. A number of the pieces of research might have been 
allotted to more than one of the categories; for instance, a particular report 
might have allocated either to the category "Social Psychology" or to the cate- 
gory of "Mental Hygiene." The purpose of the foregoing list is simply one of 
abbreviation, rather than a strictly accurate classification. 

We have thought that the readers of the Proceedings would be interested in 
seeing a list of 200 pieces of research upon which the members of the Society are 
engaged. We have prepared such a list and it is submitted at the close of this 
report. In regard to this list, it should be noted that in many cases the titles were 
not provided by the authors but have been abstracted from descriptive para- 
graphs. They will probably not in all cases be absolutely accurate, nor perhaps 
those which their authors would consider most appropriate. 

From this list of 200, the Committee selected eleven pieces of research for 
the section program. This selection was necessarily of a somewhat arbitrary 
character, the Committee being governed by considerations of variety and inter- 
est as well as those of quality of the work. 

Respectfully submitted, 

WILLIAM F. OGBURN, Chairman 
J. L. GILLIN 
W. S. THOMPSON 

SOCIAL RESEARCH IN PROGRESS, 1924 

Rural Sociology- 
Adaptation of farm families to typical regions Coon, B. F. 
Advantages of farm life Saw tele, E. H. 

American agricultural life Institute of Social and Religious Research. 
Country life in Germany, Denmark, and France Branson, E. C. 
Country town, social and economic relations of, in Ohio North, C. C. 
Country town, social and economic relations of, in Minnesota Hoffer, 

C.R. 
Country town, social and economic relations of, in New York Melvin, 

B.L. 

Country town, social and economic relations of, in Wisconsin Kolb, J. H. 
Farm housing and farm home conditions in Texas Garnett, W. E. 



COMMITTEE ON SOCIAL RESEARCH 205 

Farm housing and farm home conditions in Virginia Daggett, A. B.,and 

Pierce, J. B. 

Farm indebtedness Gillette, J. M. 

Farm population of eight selected counties Galpin, C. J., and Larson, V. B. 
Farmers' co-operative marketing, social aspects of Landis, B. 
Farmers' co-operatives, psychological and social aspects of Zimmerman, 

C. C. 
General hospitals for farm communities, the social aspects of Nason, 

W. C. 

Kinsman tenant Rankin, J. O. 
Part ownership, some social effects of Rankin, J. O. 
Population movements to and from the farm in Missouri Morgan, E. L. 
Population movements to and from the farm in Kansas Burr, Walter. 
Population movements to and from the farm Larson, V. B. 
Radio on Nebraska farms Rankin, J. O. 
Recreation survey of two rural Ohio counties Lively, C. E. 
Relation of rural community organization to type of agriculture Lively, 

C. E. 

Rural school efficiency in Kalamazoo County Burnham, E. W. 
Rural migration and other sources of urban increase, 1910-20 Gillette, 

J. M. 
Sickness and death on the life of the farm family, social and economic 

effect of Sanderson, D. L. 

Study of the best rural institutions in New Jersey Keller, H. 
Study of the best rural institutions in Illinois Whit taker, M. L. 
Study of the best rural institutions in Texas Garnett, W. E. 
Study of the best rural institutions in Virginia Gee, W. 
Study of the best rural institutions in Washington Yoder, F. R. 
Taxation and rural human welfare Rankin, J. O. 
Village and town planning Mason, W. C. 
Social Psychology 

Attitudes of farmers toward co-operative marketing Taylor, C. C. 

Creation of morals in groups Zeleney, L. D. 

Differential reactions of unions to social and economic pressure, Rhode 

Island and Nebraska Phelps, H. A. 
Dynamic aspects of social psychology French, A. N. 
Hotel life and human nature Hayner, N. S. 

Investigation of the experimental modification of attitudes Sturges, H. A. 
K.K.K. in different statesStewart, M. E. 
Literature as a means of stimulating interest on the part of the public 

Townsend, M. 

Maladjusted college student, the Harper, E. B. 
Order of birth and personalities Ogburn, W. F. 
Psychoanalysis of the Lutheran church Snyder, H. M. 



206 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

Psychological causes of historical reform movements Davis, J. 

Social possibilities of mental measurements Barry, A. G. 

Statistical study of secondary contacts radio, newspaper, etc., in 1,000 

homes in Michigan Carr, L. J. 
Verbal stimuli, differential reactions to Hart, H. 
Verbal stimuli, reaction to, and success in college Shuttleworth, F. 
What the patrons of the C.G.W.R.R. are thinking about Hoverstad, 

T. A. 

What worries boys Burger, W. H. 
Youth movement, the psychology of Aubrey, E. E. 
Standard of Living 

Cost-consumption units Brooks, M. 

Cost of living in farm houses in several areas of Iowa Atwater, H. W., 

and Kilpatrick. 
Cost of living in farmhouses in several areas of Kentucky, Tennessee, and 

Kansas Atwater, H. W., and Kilpatrick. 
Cost of living in farmhouses in Mason County, Kentucky Atwater, H. W., 

and Kilpatrick. 

Farmers' standard of living in Alabama B rooks, M. 
Farmers' standard of living in Connecticut Davis, I. G. 
Farmers' standard of living in Iowa Von Turgeln, G. 
Farmers' standard of living in Kansas Burr, W. 
Farmers' standard of living in Kentucky Nicholls, W. D. 
Farmers' standard of living in Ohio Melvin, B. L. 
Farmers' standard of living in Missouri Morgan, E. L. 
Farmers' standard of living in Nebraska Rankin, J. O. 
Farmers' standard of living in New York Sanderson, D. 
Rural standards of living (Andrews, B. R.) 1 
Standard of living of families in dairy farms in a typical hill section of 

southern New York Dickey, j . A. 

Standard of living and population pressure in China Dittmer, C. G. 
Community Life 

City of Tacoma, survey Fyprig, C. W. 

County of Kansas surveyed in the interest of child welfare Traut, G. W. 

Effect of community organizations in New York City Bowman, E. L. 

Functioning of rural community halls in New York State Felton, R. A. 

History of Death Valley Coolidge, M. R. 

Individual ascendency in rural environment Lively, C. E. 

Isolated Mormon village Nelson, L. 

Merchandising in rural communities Hoffer, C. R. 

Progress of playground movement in Indiana since 1915 Newdon, V. 

1 Names in parentheses signify part authorship, usually as part of a survey made 
by some organization. 



COMMITTEE ON SOCIAL RESEARCH 207 

Public school in the community recreation program of Pennsylvania, place 
of the Bernard, M. 

Rural recreation training schools Felton, R. A. 

Scoring of communities on the basis of the functioning of the social activi- 
ties and organizations Elmer, M. C. 

Social study of a rural New England township Hypes, J. L. 

Study of recreational facilities Community Council of St. Louis, Missouri. 

Utah type of agricultural village Nelson, L. 

What are we doing to boys? Burges, W. H. 

Dependency 

Adoption of children in Philadelphia County, Pennsylvania, Procedure of 

Deardoff, N. R. 

Care of the aged by Boston social agencies Eaves, L. 
Causal factors in dependency: study in partial correlation Chapin, F. S. 
Children left fatherless in Philadelphia: for a six-month period, study of 

Deardoff, N. R. 
Cost of public support and care of the paupers, defectives, and criminals 

in Wisconsin, 1922 Gillin, J. L. 

County almshouses in Georgia, study of Mathews, H. J. 
Dependency index for Minneapolis Chapin, F. S. 
Family desertion in Dane County, Wisconsin, study of Gillin, J. L. 
Illegitimacy cases in Dane County Wisconsin, 1912-21, study of settlements 

in Gillin, J. L. 

Outdoor relief cases in Georgia counties Mathews, J. H. 
Wage-earning women, a legacy to Eaves, L. 
Education 

Adult education, investigation and experiment in Kingsbury, S. B. 

Consolidated school districts, social aspects of Mumford, E. 

Curricular research in colleges, lack of and need for Eldridge, S. 

Educational opportunity as affected by some land problems Rankin, J. C. 

Endowments and foundations Clow, F. R. 

Historical textbooks and international differences Taft, D. R. 

Negro education in the United States, the evolution of Johnson, C. D. 

School children, how they spend time out of school Clow, F. R. 

Teaching citizenship in the schools of the United States. Bowden, A. 0. 

Tenancy and education, relationship of Rankin, J. O. 

Race 

Changes in population and farm tenure in counties of the south by color 

Work, M. N. 

Community of Indian-negro-white crossed in Virginia MacDougle, I. 
Genius by nationality groups in Who's Who Miller, A. L. 
Lynching and the race problem, 1882-1924 Work, M. N. 
Negro, the Dowd, J. 



208 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

Race relations survey Park, R. E. 

Racial forces in community life Wessel, B. B. 

Second-generation Orientals in America Smith, W. C. 

What can the schools do in assisting the efficiency of the Negro? Work, 

M. N. 
Population and Migration 

Americanization of the Finns Wargelin, J. 

City drift and its social consequences Miller, A. L. 

Demography of Champaign-Urbana Sutherland, E. H. 

Ethnic factors in the population of New London, Connecticut Wessel, 

B. B. 

Immigrants and their children Carpenter, N. 
Mexican population in a typical mid- western small city Burr, W, 
Residential mobility Sutherland, E. H. 
R61e of the village in New York State Melvin, B. L. 
Statistical aspect of immigration and population problems Carpenter, N. 

Miscellaneous and Studies of Social Work 

Agencies for disabled ex-service men, work done by Community Council 

of St. Louis. 
Contributions of Dane County income taxpayers to charity, religion, etc., 

1922 Gillin, J. L. 

Hospital beds, a study of Community Council of St. Louis. 
Nurse in industry, the functions of the Moore, Mrs. S. P. 
Negro housing in Dallas Scott, E. 

Practical service ratios of urban social agencies Elliot, Mabel 
Recreation to character building, relation of (Grady, J.) 
Unit of comparison in intensive studies Sheffield, A. E. 
Wealth of a county to the amount of welfare work, relation of the 

Mathews, J. H. 

Marriage and the Family 

Case study of several hundred families as a social institution (Grady, J .) 

Family adjustments, problems of Groves, E. R. 

Family disorganization: an introduction to sociological analysis Mowrer, 
E. R. 

Family disruption, a study of Holland, W. W. 

Family research (Holbrook, D. H.) 

Home, social conditions of the Groves, E. R. 

Marriage licenses and residence Sutherland, E. H. 
Social Medicine 

Adequacy of medical service social and economic factors Davis, M. M. 

Cost of medical care, private and institutional, and its relation to budgets 
Davis, M. M. 

Crippled children in Chicago, a survey of (Loomis, F. G.) 



COMMITTEE ON SOCIAL RESEARCH 209 

Hospitals and clinics with non-medical charities, relationships of Davis, 

M. M. 
Interrelation of medical and social factors in disease, looking toward a 

medical-social terminology Davis, M. M. 
Prevalence of disease, especially of the non-incapacitating types Davis, 

M. M. 

Socialization of medicine in the United States Moore, H. H. 
Social Change 

Evolution of social classes Eldridge, S. 

Mechanism of revolution Edwards, L. P. 

Morality as a product of special evolution Hayes, E. C. 

Mutual aid in social progress Miller, A. L. 

Relative rate of change in custom and belief of modern Jews Ravitch, 

J.S. 
Synthetic study of social evolution based on objective evidences of change 

in social structure Chapin, F. S. 
Religion 

Ethical value of belief in life after death among primitive people Wallis, 

W. D. 
Mormonism as a factor in the social, political, and educational life of North 

America Dick, V. 
Primitive religion Holland, W. W. 

Relation of the Christian religion to the economic problem Carpenter, N. 
Religious education as developed in colleges and universities Boyer, E. S. 
Sociological study of the history of religion Aubrey, E. E. 

Juvenile Delinquency 

Juvenile court in Des Moines De Graff, H. O. 

Juvenile courts of North Carolina Cowper, M. O. 

Juvenile delinquency in Dane County, Wisconsin, 1918-21 Gillin, J. L. 

Juvenile delinquency in Omaha Sullinger, T. E. 

"Normal" delinquent girls Buchan, E. 

Women going out of girls' reform schools, 1915-20 Kingsbury, S. B. 
Penology 

Attitude toward the whipping post in Delaware Crooks, E. B. 

Commercialized prostitution with relation to the municipal court, and 
medical measures in Cincinnati, 1920-24 Van Buskirk, E. F. 

Honor system in "The Work House," Delaware Crooks, E. B. 

Ontario gaol theory illustrated by Kingston gaol practice Fyprig, C. W. 

Parole and probation of adult criminals in Wisconsin Gillin, J. L. 

Labor and Industry 

Employee-representative movement Metcalf, H. C. 

I.W.W. in the United States Howd, C. R. 

Rural child labor in Texas Garnett, W. E. (and others). 



210 TEE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

Woman without responsibility in industry Kingsbury, S. B. 
Women in industry in Dallas Scott, E. 
Young employed girl Kingsbury, S. B. 

Social Economics 

Chain store a study in the ecological organization of the modern city 

Shideler, E. H. 
Distribution of products and effect of advertising and selling Lewis, 

E. St. E. 
Fundamental factors in Euro-American and Roman imperialism Spyk- 

man, V. 
Relationship between a community and its chief industry, cotton Cowper, 

M.O. 
Women in industry in St. Paul Elmer, M. C. 

Social Politics 

Five hundred and fifty measures on ballots analyzed King, J. 

Political measurements, their possibilities Eldridge, S. 

Progress in political movements in the northwest agricultural states 

Dick, V. 
Rural municipalities Manny, T. B. 

Mental Hygiene 

Admission to mental hospitals Jarrett, M. C. 
Feeblemindedness of a mid-western town of 3,500 Brooks, M. M. 
Twenty-five first-year immigrant families Jarrett, M. C. 
State care of mental hygiene in Illinois Burke, W. W. 

Law and Sociology 

Functional study of American law with particular reference to preventive 

justice Pound, R. 

Laws, state and federal, relating to opium Chamberlain, J. P. 
Noteworthy changes in statute law Chamberlain, J. P. 

Social Control 

Causes and treatment of social radicalism Reed, E. F. 

Facts that have a bearing on the probable future of war Hayes, E. C. 

Intimate devices for social control gossip, satire, etc. Lumley, F. E. 

Personality Studies 

History of the women pioneers of the Pacific West Coolidge, M. R. 
Personality studies from life-history documents Krueger, E. T, 
Story of the rise of 100 best negro farmers Caruthers, T. 

Sociological Theory 

Developments hi German sociology Mez, J. 

Social process Books taber, P. D. 

Some social relations restated Hayes, E. C. 



COMMITTEE ON AN ENCYCLOPEDIA 211 

Heredity 

Familial differential fecundity Hart, H. 

Families of Baylor College graduates Johnson, G. B. 

Intelligence tests of different races in relation to the size of family 

Kirkpatrick, C. L. 
Method 

The map as a statistical device in sociological research Mowrer, E. 
Ethics 

Statistical ethics Brogan A. P. 
Social Origins 

The genesis of social institutions Carpenter, N. 



ANNUAL REPORT OF THE COMMITTEE ON 
SOCIAL ABSTRACTS 

The Committee on Social Abstracts recommends: (i) that the Committee 
on Social Abstracts be made a standing committee of the American Sociological 
Society; (2) that the Committee consider the problem of classification of the 
literature of sociology which it is important to abstract; (3) that the Committee 
be empowered to co-operate with the Social Science Research Council in con- 
nection with its plans for a more adequate social science abstract service. 

Respectfully submitted, 

U. G. WEATHERLY 

ROBERT E. PARK 

F. STUART CHAPIN, Chairman 



REPORT OF THE COMMITTEE ON AN ENCYCLOPEDIA 
. OF THE SOCIAL SCIENCES 

Following the appointment of the committee of the Sociological Society, 
conferees were designated by the Economic, Statistical, Anthropological, and 
Political Science associations. Later individual representatives from the social 
workers and the Historical Society were added. 

At first an attempt was made to do business by letter. Several valuable 
responses with advice were received. Members of the American Sociological 
Society were also asked to respond to a questionnaire indicating their attitudes 
with regard to the proposed work. A summary of these responses indicates that 
75 per cent of those replying believe that such publication is desirable. The 
responses show that a work dealing with the general principles of allied fields is 
desired. A majority express the wish that the book should be published in four 
or six compact volumes with a dictionary or encyclopedic arrangement of topics. 
Those who expressed an opinion upon the cost of publication considered that 



212 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

both subscriptions and subsidies would be necessary to put forth the work. 
There was also general agreement that it should be published under the auspices 
of the various societies. It was further indicated that the editors would prob- 
ably have to be paid a salary and the contributors remunerated at a piece rate. 

In order to secure concurrent action upon these points a meeting of repre- 
sentatives in or near New York was held there on October 20, 1924. At this 
meeting it was voted to recommend a handbook not exceeding six volumes to 
include "non-alphabetically arranged topical articles grouped under logical 
headings." It was also recommended that an effort be made to inform the Na- 
tional Council of Social Research of the plans and endeavor to secure their co- 
operation and financial backing. 

On December 22, 1924, the New York group again met and appointed a 
subcommittee consisting of E. F. Gay, A. Goldenweiser, and Mary Van Kleeck 
to make recommendations of the Joint Committee for an executive committee 
of seven composed of one member from each of the seven associations whose 
co-operation is expected. The Joint Committee made the following recommen- 
dations to each of the co-operating associations and requested action at the 
Christmas sessions: 

1. That each association vote to continue its co-operation either by con- 
tinuing the present committee or by appointing successors. 

2. If in the next few months the Joint Committee, through its executive 
committee, reaches an agreement as to plan for co-operation, it is desirable that 
the governing body of each co-operating association should be empowered to 
ratify the plan, and action giving them such power is hereby requested. 

3. In working out preliminary details modest expenditures are necessary, 
and to cover them each society is asked to appropriate the sum of one hundred 
dollars. 

After discussing these recommendations, the Sociological Society, at its 
morning session in Chicago, December 30, 1924, voted to continue the present 
committee and also to empower the Executive Committee of the Society to 
act upon the other two recommendations. 

We have been informed that favorable action upon these points was taken 
by the American Statistical Association at the same time. 
Respectfully submitted, 
H. B. WOOLSTON, For the Sociological Committee 



REPORT OF THE COMMITTEE ON THE TEACH- 
ING OF SOCIAL STUDIES IN THE SCHOOLS 

Freedom in the Teaching of Social Science 

The teacher of social problems is peculiarly near to the volcanoes of emotion 
which impede the erection of scientific beliefs. Religious, sexual, economic, ra- 
cial, national, and political conflicts underlie almost all of the problems with 
which he is concerned. If he is to fulfil his function as a pioneer of scientific 



COMMITTEE ON THE TEACHING OF SOCIAL STUDIES 213 

method in the study of human relations he will have to use the utmost of toler- 
ance and skill in dealing with the questions peculiar to his special field. 

Recent years have intensified the difficulty which confronts teachers of so- 
cial studies in almost every line and in almost every school. Specific instances 
of exclusion of textbooks, of dismissal of teachers because of their beliefs, and of 
legislative and administrative prohibition of the teaching or discussion of cer- 
tain topics have come to the attention of this committee from Pennsylvania, 
South Carolina, Georgia, Kentucky, Louisiana, Oklahoma, Texas, Kansas, 
Missouri, and Minnesota. These are, of course, by no means the only states 
where such intolerance has been in evidence. Most of the cases noted have had 
to do with evolution and with the negro problem, but the issue is far broader 
than these two topics. 

Antagonism to the teaching of evolution is not an isolated phenomenon, but 
is allied to the general tendency toward intensely emotional attitudes toward 
religious, patriotic, racial, and industrial problems as exemplified in the Funda- 
mentalist movement, in certain activities of the American Legion, in the Ku 
Klux Klan, in the Herrin massacre, and in kindred phenomena. The solution 
demanded must be adequate not only to the issue of evolution but to cope also 
with emotional prejudices involved in the discussion of the other burning issues 
of our civilization. 

The problem of freedom of thought and freedom of teaching is not an 
ephemeral one, nor one to be ignored. Certainly the history of science gives no 
ground for the belief that the conflict between emotional prejudice and dispas- 
sionate research is a trivial or negligible element in human progress and retro- 
gression. Teachers of social studies (who in our day must bear the brunt of this 
conflict) may well endeavor to formulate and to apply sociological principles 
related to the attitudes which they should take on emotionally prejudiced 
topics. The presentation of even a crude statement of ideals may help to clarify 
the issue and to lead many of us who are teachers of sociology to a nearer ap- 
proach toward that combination of courage, tact, and pedagogical skill which 
seems essential in the struggle for truth. 

The true teacher must be faithful to the scientific spirit, no matter what 
the cost. That spirit is the open-minded love of truth; it is intellectual honesty, 
stripped of all preconceived prejudices; it is an eager, unflincing, unflagging 
quest which follows wherever truth may lead. It abhors that venal opportunism 
which acquiesces with popular views irrespective of one's personal conceptions 
of truth and of scientific progress, so as to produce the best-selling textbooks 
and so as to procure maximum personal advancement. 

Yet sociologists must recognize that direct conflict is not always the best 
method to achieve their end. The data of social psychology are replete with 
evidence as to the dangers involved in antagonism as a means of social control. 
Open conflict too often reinforces the very emotional attitudes which are being 
opposed. Coercion has been notoriously unsuccessful as a method of assimilat- 
ing racial and religious groups. The sociologist may well query whether his own 
science justifies the attempt to conquer intolerant groups by aggressive attack. 



214 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

As scientists we must adopt the objective pragmatic attitude; we must seek the 
answer to this question: By what type of policy can the sociologist best promote 
in the rising generation the love of truth and the fearless and dispassionate search 
for it? The problem becomes not an emotional but an intellectual issue. The 
seeker may arrive at some such policy as the following set of principles implies: 

1. As far as is consistent with his function as a guide in the search for truth, 
the teacher of sociology should adopt, in presenting the facts essential to his 
subject, those methods which arouse the least emotional antagonism: (a) He 
should refrain from denouncing specifically the treasured verbal formulas in 
which the individuals whom he desires to influence have been accustomed to 
embody their beliefs, their hopes, and their loyalties, (b) In discussing contro- 
versial subjects, he should avoid making sweeping generalizations in which oc- 
cur words which have become symbols for emotional conflict, such as "evolu- 
tion," "God," "miracles," "socialism," "capitalistic," "Jew," "negro," "Amer- 
ica," sex terms and the like, (c) The teacher of sociology should avoid implying 
that persons who disagree with him are insincere, dishonest, stupid, or inferior. 
(d) In the preparation of textbooks and in the presentation of courses, the so- 
ciologist should avoid conflict on trivial and non-essential matters and on sub- 
jects remote from the immediate problems to be dealt with. 

2. In attempting to modify unscientific attitudes, the teacher should bring 
to the attention of the student the facts which are inconsistent with his false 
position and urge him himself without bias to account for these facts. He should 
do his utmost to persuade the student to discover the truth for himself rather 
than to convey to the student the teacher 's conclusions. 

3. The sociologist should use his whole influence in favor of impartial dis- 
cussion and freedom of speech, especially insisting upon a fair hearing for his 
opponents. The teacher should present fully the facts which are favorable to 
the side in which he does not believe, as well as those favorable to his own posi- 
tion, and point out their possible interpretations. 

4. If the sociologist finds that his subject cannot adequately be presented 
without arousing emotional antagonism he should nevertheless go forward fear- 
lessly and take the consequences. As a teacher he should endeavor conscien- 
tiously, courageously, and ardently to encourage in his students and his readers 
the eager and impartial search for truth. No consideration for his own safety, 
advancement, or financial advantage should deter him from this purpose. 

In the long run, who need fear for the outcome as between science and super- 
stition? As dedicated servants of an invincible cause, let us never allow our 
selfish interests to dilute our loyalty to truth, nor our enthusiastic impatience to 
interfere with the independence of thought and the gradual wholesome growth 
of the minds intrusted to our guidance. 



The discussion of the foregoing memorandum at the luncheon at which it 
was presented brought out some points which should be noted here. Certain 
speakers felt that a direct challenge to the emotional prejudices of the students 



COMMITTEE ON THE TEACHING OF SOCIAL STUDIES 215 

had a valuable stimulating effect. An instance was cited in which a teacher who, 
it was stated, had been attacked and left for dead by the Ku Klux Klan, had, 
thereupon, a tremendous and permanent increase in the enrolment of his classes. 
In connection with giving a fair hearing to the opposition some of those present 
at the luncheon felt that this was likely to result in having the time of the class 
wasted by verbose faddists; it was suggested that the teacher necessarily must 
exercise a selective function. Still another speaker doubted whether it was ad- 
visable to attempt to recommend any one method of teaching. 



Can we Measure Results in Teaching Social Science? 

Before scientific conclusions can be reached as to the relative merits of 
various methods of teaching social science some method must be devised for 
measuring the attitudes which such teaching seeks to produce. 

Experiments with a test called the "personnel assay er," as administered to 
525 representative fifteen-year-old school children and to delinquents in cer- 
tain institutions have yielded results looking toward the measurement of 
delinquency and of socialization. 

The typical school child, according to this investigation, is intolerant, but 
not to the extent of approving mob violence. For example, of the 525 public- 
school children tested, 71 per cent agreed that "it is wicked to teach that there 
are things in the Bible that are not true"; on the other hand, 83 per cent in- 
dorsed the statement that "it is a very dangerous and evil practice for mobs to 
take it on themselves to enforce the law." 

Culture conceit and ethnic egotism characterize this sample of American 
youth. That "most jews try to get the best of a bargain even if they have to 
cheat to do it" was the opinion of 70 per cent, and 80 per cent felt that the lives 
of the best Chinese are worth no more to the world than the lives of average 
American laborers. That "no country ever had as good laws or as good a gov- 
ernment, or a culture as good in any way as we people of the United States 
have" was asserted by 80 per cent of the children. National isolation was the 
favored policy. Dislike for war was expressed by 90 per cent, but the majority 
thought that war would continue indefinitely to be as common as in the past 
few centuries. 

The quality of the patriotism taught these children is suggested by the 
fact that the sentence "Good patriots are always loyal to their political party" 
was indorsed by 75 per cent. A strong interest in "reverence for Old Glory" was 
shown by 60 per cent of the children, but only 19 per cent showed a correspond- 
ingly strong interest in "work for social justice." 

Majorities believed that "crime can best be reduced by punishing every 
criminal severely," and that "all strikes should be prohibited"; 43 per cent 
favored prohibiting smoking tobacco. In the eyes of these youngsters smoking 
by women and the falseness of a friend were about equally reprehensible. 

Reactions of girls indicated more sensitiveness, more interest in religion, and 
less interest in economic and social questions than the boys showed. Delin- 



216 TEE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

quent boys rated by their teachers as brutal or unkind reacted to the test in 
ways which indicated defiance of conventions and lack of sympathy and ideal- 
ism. Particularly striking was their antagonism toward religious stimuli. On 
the other hand, delinquents without marked brutal tendencies were likely to 
pretend to be very intellectual, very sympathetic, and especially very religious. 
The "pretender" group is apparently much more numerous among delinquents 
than are the "defiers." 

Using these reactions as a basis, a tentative scoring method has been devised 
which is highly reliable for identifying the pretender type of delinquent. This 
method is quite complex, however, and will not be put into circulation until 
further experiments have been carried out. 

HORNELL HART, Chairman 
Ross L. FINNEY 
H. A. MOORE 



REPORT OF THE COMMITTEE ON INTERNATIONAL 

RELATIONS 

At the meeting two years ago a committee on international relations was 
appointed with the definite commission to investigate the relation of news to 
international public opinion and to undertake such further tasks as it saw fit. 
As I reported last year, it seemed wise, for the purpose of carrying more weight 
and perhaps more easily to secure financial support, that the news-investigation 
project be submitted to the Social Science Research Council with the request 
that it assume the responsibility. This was done last February and the Council 
agreed to undertake the investigation through a specially appointed committee. 
The project as defined and accepted by the Council is as follows: "A thorough 
scientific and objective investigation of the instrumentalities involved in the 
world-wide collection and dissemination of current news and opinion of inter- 
national concern, and of the underlying and related problems of the formation 
and expression and significance of attitudes on international affairs." 

Money was secured for a preliminary conference for the careful planning 
of the undertaking. At the first meeting of the committee, it was assumed that 
the committee itself should create an organization for carrying out the work, 
probably assigning certain portions to various institutions. 

At a recent meeting of the committee it was decided that it would be more 
valuable if, before such an organization were formed, the widest possible advice 
and co-operation should be secured. The farther we proceed the more clear it 
becomes that the ramifications of this undertaking are very extensive, and that 
there are many factors which affect public opinion which need to be understood 
before it will be possible to analyze the influence of news; and it may be found 
that considerable work has been done on parts of this, and that there may be 
special fields which individuals or institutions might be glad to undertake, and 



PERSONNEL CLASSIFICATION IN FEDERAL GOVERNMENT 217 

still other special aspects which individuals would be glad to finance who would 
not be willing to put a very large sum into a general investigation. 

At the present time a preliminary outline of the project as it is now seen is 
being prepared. This will very shortly be sent out widely for criticism and sug- 
gestions. After these have been received, further advice will be sought from 
various foreign countries. Many of you will be asked to help with the criticism 
and suggestions in the near future. It is quite obvious that this work will cover 
several years, and there seems to be no difference of opinion as to its very funda- 
mental and important character. 

The only other activity of the committee has been to make some overtures 
in the direction of international co-operation with social scientists, but since the 
field of sociology is not sharply defined abroad, it seems wise to the chairman 
of this committee that the political scientists and economists should join with 
the sociologists for this purpose. The matter has been presented to both asso- 
ciations, and something of this sort may be done at their current meetings. 

Committee on International Relations, 
H. A. MILLER, Chairman 



REPORT OF THE COMMITTEE ON PERSONNEL 

CLASSIFICATION IN THE FEDERAL 

GOVERNMENT 

The American Statistical Association, the American Economic Association, 
the American Sociological Society, and the American Association for Labor 
Legislation at their annual meetings in December, 1923, appointed committees 
to present to the President of the United States, to the Civil Service Committees 
of the two Houses of Congress and to the Federal Personnel Classification Board 
briefs regarding the proper classification of scientific positions in economics, 
sociology, and statistics and to take such other action as might prove desirable 
to bring about the proper classification of such positions. 

The Budget for the fiscal year 1925 had revealed the fact that the Person- 
nel Classification Board had generally classified scientific government positions 
in the fields of economics, sociology, and statistics as being in the "Clerical 
Administrative and Fiscal Service" as defined in the Classification Act of 1923, 
and not in the "Professional and Scientific Service" as defined in that Act. 

The Associations are directly interested in this action of the Personnel 
Classification Board, because persons working in the social sciences are largely 
dependent on government sources for the data used in arriving at decisions on 
economic and social matters and because the quality of the government's work 
in these fields is determined principally by the qualifications of the persons 
doing the government work. 

The Classification Act of 1923 recognized the necessity for paying larger 
salaries for professional and scientific positions in the government service in order 



2i8 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

to attract and retain properly qualified workers. Had the scientific positions 
in economics, sociology, and statistics been allocated to the Professional and 
Scientific Service they would have been assigned to salary grades on the basis 
of the definitions prescribed by Congress to apply to professional and scientific 
positions. These definitions are entirely different from the definitions for the 
clerical, administrative, and fiscal services. The use of the professional and 
scientific grade definitions would have resulted in offering for economists, 
sociologists, and statisticians compensation more nearly comparable to that 
offered by private enterprise. At the same time standards for entrance into 
these positions would have been established on the basis of professional and 
scientific qualifications so that the government would have derived the benefits 
sought by Congress in increasing the rates of compensation for professional and 
scientific workers. 

Formal protest against the action of the Personnel Board is made both on 
the ground that it is contrary to the letter and the spirit of the Classification 
Act and that it is against the interests of the country which require more com- 
plete and intelligent application of the social sciences. 

The Classification Act of 1923 appeared to be a step forward for two reasons: 
it provided a much needed adjustment of salaries for government employees, 
more particularly for the professional, scientific, and technical employees; and 
having provided more adequate salaries, it permitted improving the system for 
securing properly qualified employees. It was to be the instrument for giving 
full effect to the sound fundamental principles of the merit system. Recognition 
of the scientific, technical nature of large numbers of governmental positions 
and the establishment of proper entrance requirements for them were primary 
steps in improving the service. In no fields were they more important than in 
economics, sociology, and statistics. The Classification Act was to have been a 
means of giving to the people a higher degree of efficiency and usefulness in the 
application of the social sciences in the solution of public problems. The action 
of the Personnel Classification Board to a large extent negatives the Act. 

On behalf of the associations which we represent we therefore urge that the 
scientific character of the work in the fields of economics, sociology, and sta- 
tistics be recognized, that positions in these fields requiring technical or sci- 
entific training equivalent to graduation from a college or university of recog- 
nized standing be allocated to the Professional and Scientific Service, and that 
the salaries of positions so allocated be fixed in accord with the definitions of the 
several salary grades established by the Act for that service. 

CARL KELSEY 
University of Pennsylvania 

ROBERT R. KEHN 
George Washington University 

MOLLDE R. CARROLL 
Goucher College 



REPORTS OF REPRESENTATIVES OF THE SOCIETY 
TO THE SOCIAL-SCIENCE RESEARCH COUNCIL 

The Social-Science Research Council now consists of three representatives, 
each from the American Political Science Association, the American Sociological 
Society, the American Economic Association, and the American Statistical 
Association, appointed for terms of three years. 

The Social-Science Research Council was organized in 1923, in accordance 
with a concurrent resolution setting up as the purpose of the organization that 
of "promoting and co-ordinating research, and furthering the development of 
research methods in the social studies." During the year 1924 four sessions 
have been held, February 16, May 17, November 29, and December 31, and 
substantial progress has been made in various directions. 

1. Research Fellowships 

A plan for research fellowships has been worked out by a committee of the 
Council of which Professor A. B. Hall was chairman, and adopted by the 
Council. This plan provides for granting opportunities for study of social prob- 
lems analogous to those now given to students of natural science, and it is 
believed will greatly stimulate research and make possible significant advances. 
A copy of the plan is attached hereto. The Fellowship Committee of three 
elected by the Council consists of Professor Wesley C. Mitchell of Columbia 
University, chairman, Professor F. S. Chapin of the University of Minnesota, 
secretary, and Professor Charles E. Merriam of the University of Chicago. It 
is hoped that substantial support of the work of this committee will be available 
for the year 1925-26. 

2. Committee on Scientific Aspects of Human Migration 

Early in 1924 the National Research Council's Committee on Scientific 
Aspects of Human Migration requested the Social Science Research Council to 
appoint a similar committee on the social aspects of the problem. It was the 
intention that these two committees, while independent financially and tech- 
nically, should co-operate as closely as possible in preparing a program for the 
study of migration, and in the scientific study of the projects outlined. 

The principal project undertaken by our committee during this year has 
been the study of the mechanization of industry in relation to migration (in the 
United States). This was in charge of Professor W. C. Mitchell of Columbia 
and Professor Harry Jerome of the University of Wisconsin. A gift of $15,500 
was made for this purpose, and the inquiry is now well under way. The Com- 
mittee has worked on a series of plans for the study of migration during the year 
and has made progress in the development of a significant program. Among the 

219 



220 TEE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

more important of the features of the program as thus far discussed are a statis- 
tical survey of world migrations, an inquiry into Canadian immigration laws, a 
study of the immigration problem in Argentina, a study of personality and race 
differences. The members of this Committee are Edith Abbot, chairman, Pro- 
fessor Ogburn, Professor Ogg, Professor John R. Commons, Professor Wesley C. 
Mitchell, Professor John A. Fairlie, Professor H. A. Miller, Professor Wittke, 
Professor R. M. Yerkes, Professor Wissler, Mary Van Kleek. 

3. International News and Communication 

During the year a project for a study of international news and communica- 
tion was referred to the Committee, and after consideration the Council ap- 
proved of a "thoroughly scientific and objective investigation of the instru- 
mentalities involved in the world-wide gathering and dissemination of current 
news and opinion of international concern, and of the underlying related prob- 
lems of the formation, expression, and significance of attitudes on international 
affairs." 

The Committee consists of Walter S. Rogers, chairman, Mr. Franklin 
Adams, Pan-American Union, Professor Willard G. Bleyer, School of Journal- 
ism, University of Wisconsin, Dean Walter Williams, School of Journalism, 
University of Missouri, Dr. Edwin W. Slosson, National Research Council 
Science Service, Professor Harold G. Moulton, director, Institute of Economics, 
Professor Jerome Davis, Yale University, Professor R. E. Park, University of 
Chicago, Mr. Bruce Bliven, New York, Professor George G. Wilson, Harvard 
University, Professor R. M. Yerkes, Yale University, Professor W. F. Og- 
burn, Columbia University, H. A. Miller, Ohio State University. The Com- 
mittee has held several sessions and is preparing a careful project for the study 
of the problem as stated by the Council. The sum of $2,500 has been contributed 
for the purpose of forming this plan. 

4. Publication of Index and Digest of State Legislation 

This Committee, of which Professor J. P. Chamberlin of Columbia is 
chairman, has continued the effort of the previous year to obtain support for 
the annual preparation and publication of an index and digest of all state legis- 
lation by the United States government through a congressional appropriation. 
In addition to the indorsement of the plan given by the constituent members 
of the Council, many other indorsements have been obtained and substantial 
progress has been made toward the end in view. The undoubtedly large useful- 
ness of such a publication to all those interested in any form of social research 
makes the advance of the Committee's work of great significance to the Council 
and to all the members of the constituent societies as well as to large numbers 
of other persons and organizations. 

5. Committee on Social-Science Abstracts 

This Committee, of which Professor F. S. Chapin is chairman, reported the 
publication of short notes on three alternative plans for abstracting social- 



REPORTS OF REPRESENTATIVES 221 

science periodical literature which appeared during the summer in the journals 
of the societies of the constituent members of the Council. The recommenda- 
tion of the Committee is attached hereto. 

6. Committee on Survey of Social-Science Agencies in the United States (Professor 
Secristj Chairman) 

This Committee plans a survey of the most significant social-science agen- 
cies in the United States with a view of ascertaining types of available material 
for social research, and also for the purpose of scrutinizing and comparing the 
various types of method employed by these agencies. Such an inquiry would 
provide basic data of the most important nature and would lead to an intensive 
scrutiny of the problems of method in the field of social research. The Com- 
mittee has formulated a plan of inquiry, but thus far has not been able to obtain 
funds for the prosecution of the undertaking. 

REPORT OF COMMITTEE ON SOCIAL-SCIENCE ABSTRACTS 

TO THE SOCIAL-SCIENCE RESEARCH COUNCIL, 

NOVEMBER 29, 1924 

1. We recommend the restablishment of some plan of central editorial 
supervision (co-operative) for social-science abstracts such as outlined in the 
recent statement published in the social-science journals. Plan I is recom- 
mended as desirable, but if funds cannot be obtained, Plan II is recommended 
as an alternative. 

Plan I. Publication of social-science abstracts for economics, political 
science, sociology, and statistics, as a separate monthly or quarterly bulletin. 

a) Expense to be met by subsidy from some national foundation, or by 
joint-membership dues of the societies, or by contribution from the constituent 
societies, (estimated cost, $14,600, at added expense per member of $2.18 to 
$2.44 yearly). 

b) Acceptance of guidance over abstracting by a central editorial body 
representative of the whole field, with (i) common methodology of abstracting ? 
(2) conformance to some objective system of classification of subjects, (3) cross- 
referencing, (4) editing and abstracting paid. 

Plan II. Publication of social science abstracts in separate journals as at 
present with enlargement of these services to cover the field more adequately, 
(a) expense to be met by dues increased by $2.08 yearly, (b) central editorial 
organization as described under (b) of Plan I. 

Plan HI. Publication of social-science abstracts in separate journals as at 
present with enlargement of these services to cover the field more adequately; 
(a) expense to be met by dues increased by $1.50 yearly; (b) no central editorial 
supervision, but (i) agreement on common basis of classification of abstracts, 
each journal to publish in full its own abstracts, and printing merely the scheme 
of classification of each other journal; (2) abstracts to be paid for by the page. 

2. We recommend the appointment of a standing committee on abstracts 



222 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

of periodical literature by each constituent society, (a) to co-operate in further- 
ing the plan of joint abstracting; (V) to classify the subject-matter of the special 
field of knowledge of the particular science concerned. 

F. STUART CHAPIN (Sociology), Chairman 
DAVIS R, DEWEY (Economics) 
A. C. HANFORD (Political Science) 
WALTER F. WILLCOX (Statistics) 



Other significant actions of the Council are the vote bringing to the atten- 
tion of the Association of American Universities the desirability of the general 
adoption, by universities granting the degree of Doctor of Philosophy with social 
sciences, of two measures calculated to render more easily available research 
work done in fulfilment of requirements for the doctorate, namely, (i) the main- 
tenance, under suitable restrictions, of an interlibrary loan file of doctoral dis- 
sertations in the social sciences; (2) the annual publication of abstracts of 
doctoral dissertations accepted by the university during each academic year, 
each candidate to be required to submit with his dissertation an acceptable 
abstract of it. 

With respect to the following action of the executive council of the American 
Economic Association, "To refer to the Social Science Research Council the 
question of the advisability of the American Economic Association changing 
its status in A.A.A.S. from that of 'associated' members to that of 'affiliated' 
member, and to ask the Council to consider the effect that the entrance of the 
American Economic Association into the A.A.A.S. would have on the other con- 
stituent associations," it was voted as follows: "It is believed that the constit- 
uent associations should act only in concert in dealing with the A.A.A.S., and 
that the relation of the social sciences to the A.A.A.S. should be made the matter 
of further study by the Social Science Research Council and its constituent 
members." 

The Council voted, November 29, to request the constituent members of 
the Council not to fix a time and place of meeting in 1925 without first consult- 
ing the other constituent members. This request was made in the hope that it 
might be possible at intervals, at any rate, to arrange for meetings of all the 
social sciences at the same time and place, and possibly for one or more joint 
sessions for consideration of common problems in the field of social research. 
It is strongly hoped by the Council that it may be possible to bring this about. 

F. STUART CHAPIN 
SHELBY M. HARRISON 
WILLIAM F. OGBURN 



REPORTS OF REPRESENTATIVES 223 

REPORT OF THE DELEGATES OF THE AMERICAN 

SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY TO THE AMERICAN 

COUNCIL OF LEARNED SOCIETIES 

The annual meeting of the American Council of Learned Societies was held 
in New York City on January 26, 1924. The chairman, Charles H. Haskins, re- 
ported a grant of $6,000 from the Carnegie Corporation for a survey of learned 
societies. Reports of progress were made by the Committee on a Dictionary of 
American Biography and by the other committees of the Council. Edward C. 
Armstrong, Waldo G. Leland, and Paul Shorey were selected as delegates to 
the annual meeting of the Union Academique Internationale in Brussels, May 
12-14. Waldo G. Leland was appointed director of the survey of learned so- 
cieties and began active work on the survey July i. The American Council of 
Learned Societies was incorporated in Washington October 28, and held an 
organization meeting on October 30. A revised form of the original constitution 
was submitted to the constituent organizations for ratification. At a special 
meeting of the Council, December 6, an announcement was made of the gift by 
the New York Times and its publisher, Mr. Adolph Ochs, of $500,000, payable 
over a period of ten years, for the preparation and editing of the Dictionary of 
American Biography. The permanent office of the Council was established in 
Washington and Waldo G. Leland was appointed as executive secretary for the 
year 1925. A subvention of $8,000 from the Carnegie Corporation for the work 
of the Council in 1925 was announced. 

Respectfully submitted, 

FRANKLIN H. GIDDINGS 
WILLIAM F. OGBURN, Delegates 



REPORT OF THE MEETING OF THE SECTION ON EDUCATIONAL 

SOCIOLOGY 

The section on educational sociology held two meetings during the general 
session. The first was a general meeting held at 9 : 30 Tuesday morning. It was 
presided over by the chairman of the section, Dr. Walter R. Smith, of the 
University of Kansas, and had an attendance of something over forty. Care- 
fully prepared papers were read by Dr. Franklin Bobbitt, of the University of 
Chicago, and Dr. Ira W. Howerth, of the State Teachers' College, Greeley. 
Colorado. These papers were followed by a general discussion led by Dr. F. 
Stuart Chapin, of the University of Minnesota, and Dr. David Snedden, of 
Columbia University, and participated in by a number of others. 

Dr. Bobbitt has been a pioneer in the field of curriculum reconstruction 
and the scientific determination of educational objectives by a painstaking 
analysis of social activities. His paper dealt with the contributions educators 
have a right to expect sociologists to make in redirecting education into greater 
social usefulness. Dr. Howerth has written freely in both sociology and educa- 



224 TBE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

tion and his paper contained a critical summary of the sociological bases of a 
science of education. Each of these papers will shortly be available in printed 
form, that of Dr. Howerth appearing in an early issue of the Journal of Social 
Forces, making it unnecessary, as it would be unsafe, to attempt a summary 
from memory. 

The second session was a luncheon conference of those interested in re- 
search in educational sociology. It took place at the City Club with an attend- 
ance of twenty-two, and was presided over by Dr. David Snedden. Numerous 
reports of research projects undertaken or supervised by those present were 
made. General discussions were centered upon methods of investigation, the 
sort of problems available for theses, and the need of a clearing-house through 
which researches may be assembled and made generally available. Plans were 
also discussed for the annual meeting of the national Society for the Study of 
Educational Sociology, which takes place at Cincinnati, February 25 and 26. 

The section on educational sociology had its inception in a general program 
of the American Sociological Society at its meeting in 1922. An evening session 
was devoted to education, under the chairmanship of Dr. David Snedden. Such 
was the interest aroused that a meeting was held the following February at 
Cleveland during the sessions of the Department of Superintendence of the 
National Education Association. At this meeting the National Society for the 
Study of Educational Sociology was organized. Since then two meetings a year 
have been held, one in connection with the American Sociological Society, and 
the other during the sessions of the Department of Superintendence. 

The value of two sessions per year is evident. Educational sociology is not 
only a sub-science of general sociology, but it is a basic science of education. Its 
appeal is to workers in each field, and its advancement must depend upon the 
co-operation of sociologists and educationists. These groups must somehow be 
made to understand each other, to realize that neither is in a position to patron- 
ize the other, and that any worth-while accomplishment in educational sociology 
will be mutually helpful to both. This mutuality is illustrated in the parallel 
field of educational psychology. Without doubt a large share of the progress in 
school work during the past half-century has been due to the searchlight thrown 
upon the learning process by psychologists. In return, teachers have flocked 
into psychology classrooms and students of education have made the most sig- 
nificant contributions to scientific psychological advance. In a similar way, the 
progress of education in the next half-century may be immensely furthered by 
throwing the searchlight of sociology upon the educative process, particularly 
with reference to socialization. In proportion as this is done our three-quarters 
of a million teachers will provide an enormous clientele for sociology classrooms, 
and students of education may be expected to make some of the most note- 
worthy contributions to the advancement of general sociology. Thus educa- 
tional sociologists need to affiliate themselves with both sociologists and educa- 
tionists, learning from each, and hoping to contribute to each, in equal pro- 
portions. 

WALTER R. SMITH, Chairman 



PROGRAM OF THE NINETEENTH ANNUAL MEETING, 
CHICAGO, ILLINOIS, DECEMBER 29-31, 1924 

MONDAY, DECEMBER 29 
9: oo A.M. Registration. 
10:00-12:00 A.M. Meetings of sections of the Society. 

Section on Social Research. In charge of W. F. Ogburn, Columbia Uni- 
versity. Ten-minute reports on research projects. 

"The Contributions of the Income Taxpayers of Dane County, Wiscon- 
sin, to Charity, Religion, and Education." John L. Gillin, University of 
Wisconsin. 

"Familial Differential Fecundity." Hornell Hart, Bryn Mawr College. 
"Relative Rate of Change in Custom and Belief of Modern Jews." 
Jessie Ravitch, University of Minnesota. 

"Personality Studies from Life History Documents." E. T. Krueger, 
Vanderbilt University. 

"Historical Textbooks and International Differences." Donald R. Taft, 
Wells College. 

"Some Researches in Rural Group Analysis." John H. Kolb, Univer- 
sity of Wisconsin. 

"Commodity Distribution in Rural Communities." C. R. Hoffer, Uni- 
versity of Minnesota. 

"A Measure of Rural Migration and Other Sources of Urban Increase." 
J. M. Gillette, University of North Dakota. 

"Some Tendencies and Aspects of the Race Problem, 1912-24." Monroe 
N. Work, Tuskegee Normal and Industrial Institute. 
"Standard of Living and Population Pressure in China." C. I. Dittmer, 
University of Wisconsin. 

"A Dependency Index for Minneapolis." F. Stuart Chapin, University 
of Minnesota. 

Section on Sociology of Religion. In charge of Herbert N. Shenton, Co- 
lumbia University. Club Room, Mezzanine Floor. 

The raison (Tdtre of this meeting. Charles A. Ellwood, University of Mis- 
souri. 

"Sociology of Religion." Herbert N. Shenton, Columbia University. 
"Practical Application of Sociology to Current Religious Problems." 
Justin W. Nixon, Rochester, New York. 

"Possibilities and Limitations of a Section on Religious Sociology." War- 
ren H. Wilson, Department of Church and Country Life, Presbyterian 
Board of Home Missions. 
Discussion. Leading to action determining the future of the section. 

225 



226 TEE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

12 130 P.M. Luncheon Conferences: 

Section on Rural Sociology and American Farm Economic Association. 
South Room, Ninth Floor. 

"Rural Income and Standard of Living." In charge of C. J. Galpin, Bureau 
of Agricultural Economics, Washington, D.C. 

For the economists, M. L. Wilson, Bureau of Agricultural Economics, 
Washington, D.C. For the sociologists, D wight Sanderson, Cornell Uni- 
versity. Discussion, Hildegarde Kneeland, Bureau of Home Economics, 
Washington, D.C. 

Section on the Family. In charge of Mrs. William F. Dummer. Lincoln 
Room, City Club, 315 Plymouth Court. 

"Modern Conditions Influencing the Family." Ernest R. Groves, Boston 
University 

3:00-5:00 P.M. Division on Social Psychology. In charge of Ellsworth Faris, 
University of Chicago. 

"Psychology and Culture." A. A. Goldenweiser, New School for Social 
Research. 

Discussion. L. L. Bernard, University of Minnesota. 
"Cultural Trends and Technique." Robert E. Park, University of Chi- 
cago. 

Discussion. Emory S. Bogardus, University of Southern California. 
"The Subjective Aspect of Culture." Ellsworth Faris. 
Discussion. Floyd H. Allport, Syracuse University. 

5:00 P.M. Meeting of the Executive Committee. 

8 : oo P.M. Joint Session with the American Economic Association and with the 
American Statistical Association. The Gold Room, the Congress Hotel. 
Presidents' addresses: 

"Intolerance." Charles A. Ellwood, American Sociological Society. 
"Quantitative Analysis in Economic Theory." Wesley C. Mitchell, Ameri- 
can Economic Association. 

"The Statistician and the Population Problem." Louis I. Dublin, Ameri- 
can Statistical Association. 

9:30 P.M. Smoker tendered by the business men of Chicago. 

TUESDAY, DECEMBER 30 

9: co A.M. Business meeting for the reports of committees. 
10:00-12:00 A.M. Division on Statistical Sociology. In charge of Walter F. 
Willcox, Cornell University. 

"American and European Population Densities and the Immigration Policy 
of the United States." Niles Carpenter, University of Buffalo. 
Discussion. Arthur J. Todd, Northwestern University. 
"The Need for Improved Child Welfare Statistics." R. M. Woodbury, In- 
stitute of Economics, Washington, D.C. 



PROGRAM OF THE NINETEENTH ANNUAL MEETING 227 

Discussion. Grace Abbott, Federal Children's Bureau; George B. Mangold, 
St. Louis, Missouri. 

"The Development of American Vital Statistics." Walter F. Willcox. 
Discussion. G. R. Davies, University of North Dakota. 
Section on Educational Sociology. In charge of W. R. Smith, University 
of Kansas. Club Room, Mezzanine Floor. 

"The Contribution Sociology Should Be Expected to Make to Education, 
Science, and School Procedure." Ira Howerth, Colorado State College, and 
Franklin Bobbitt, University of Chicago. Discussion led by David Sned- 
den, Columbia University and F. Stuart Chapin, University of Minnesota. 
12:30 P.M. Luncheon Conferences: 

Section on Rural Sociology. "Next Steps in Rural Social Research." South 
Room, Ninth Floor. 

A. "Emphasis Regarding 'Knowing Your Own State' and 'Making a 
Special Contribution.' " Statement: J. H. Kolb, University of Wisconsin. 
Outline: S. H. Hobbs, Jr., University of North Carolina. 

B. "Special Needs for a Social Psychological Emphasis." L. L. Bernard, 
University of Minnesota. 

Discussion. Carl C. Taylor, North Carolina State College; C. E. Lively, 

Ohio State University. 

Section on the Teaching of Social Sciences in the Public Schools. In charge 

of Hornell Hart, Bryn Mawr College. North Room, Ninth Floor. 

"Freedom in the Teaching of Social Science." 

"Can We Measure Results in Teaching Social Science?" 

Discussion limited to five minutes for each speaker. 

Section on Educational Sociology. City Club, 315 Plymouth Court. 

Round table for Discussion of Research Projects. 
3:00-5:00 P.M. Section on International Relations. In charge of Herbert A. 

Miller, Ohio State University. 

Report of the Committee on International Relations and Co-operation. 

Herbert A. Miller, chairman. 

"Surveying the Press." Walter Williams, University of Missouri. 

"The Background of News." Robert E. Park, University of Chicago. 

"The International Activities of the Soviet Government." Pitirim Sorokin, 

University of Minnesota. 

Discussion. Jerome Davis, Yale University. 

"The Sociological Factor in the Interpretation of International Relations 

with Specific Illustrations from Southeastern Europe and the Near East." 

Earle E. Eubank, University of Cincinnati. 

Discussion. Herbert A. Miller. 

2:45-5:00 P.M. Section on Rural Sociology. In charge of John H. Kolb, Uni- 
versity of Wisconsin. South Parlor, First Floor. 

"Significant Factors in Rural Population Affecting Our Civilization." 



228 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

"Farm Population." John M. Gillette, University of North Dakota. 
"Village Population." C. Luther Fry, Institute of Social and Religious 
Research, New York City. 

"Rural Demography." Warren S. Thompson, Scripps Foundation, Miami 
University. 

Discussion. W. L. Bailey, Northwestern University. 

6:30 P.M. Annual Dinner of the American Sociological Society. In honor of 
Albion W. Small and Franklin H. Giddings. Speakers: George E. Vin- 
cent and James P. Lichtenberger. South Room, Ninth Floor. 

WEDNESDAY, DECEMBER 31 

9: co A.M. Annual Business Meeting of the American Sociological Society. 
10:00-12:00 A.M. Division on Biological Factors. In charge of Frank H. 
Hankins, Smith College. 

"Race Crossing in the Light of Modern Genetics." L. C. Dunn, Storrs 
Agricultural Experiment Station. 

Discussion. Frank H. Hankins. * 

"The Hybrid as a Sociological Type." E. B. Reuter, University of Iowa. 
"An Anthropological View of Race Mixture." Ralph Linton, Field Muse- 
um, Chicago. 

Discussion. Kimball Young, University of Oregon. 

"On a Method for the Study of the Phenomenon of Nationalism." Max S. 
Handman, University of Texas. 



THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

ANNUAL REPORT OF THE SECRETARY FOR THE FISCAL YEAR 
DECEMBER i, 1923, TO NOVEMBER 30, 1924 

Membership Statement 

Last year the total membership of the Society was 1,141 ; this year it num- 
bers 1,193, a g a i n f 5 2 members. 



Membership in 1923 ....................... i , 141 

Members resigning .................... 63 

Members dropped ..................... 190 

Members deceased .................... 3 

Total lost ........................ 256 

Members renewing 

ex officio ........................... i 

exchange ........................... 5 

paid ............................... 879 

New members ........................ 308 

Total members for 1924 ............... i , 193 

Life Members 

The life members of the Society now include the following persons : Jerome 
Davis, Thomas D. Eliot, Earle E. Eubank, Ellsworth Faris, Mrs. Richard 
Ford, J. C. Harper, W. Clinton Heffner, Bertha A. Irving, Shiko Kusama, 
Samuel McC. Lindsay, Maud Loeber, Jane I. Newell, Jesus Rivero Quijano, 
Frederic Siedenburg, Teizo Toda, Arthur J. Todd, W. Russell Tylor, Christine 
Lofsted, T. C. Wang, L. D. Weyand, James O. Welchel, Frederic G. Young. 

Recommendation for Membership 

Among those sending in lists of applications or recommended persons are 
Floyd Allport, Alice Belcher, Caroline Bengston, F. W. Blackmar, Starr 
Cadwallader, Grace E. Chaffee, C. H. Cooley, E. H. Davis, E. E. Eubank, R. W. 
Frank, Henry S. French, Charles J. Galpin, John L. Gillin, E. R. Groves, 
June Purcell-Guild, Clara E. Howard, Susan M. Kingsbury, J. H. Kolb, D. C. 
Kulp II, F. E. Lumley, R. D. McKenzie, O. O. Norris, E. B. Reuter, H. N. 
Shenton, W. C. Smith, Ellwood Street, H. H. Strong, W. F. Willcox, H. B. 
Woolston. 

Interest of Members in Divisions of the Society 

In 1924 for the first time members were invited to indicate the divisions of 
the Society in which they were most interested. Of the 1,193 members of the 
Society less than one-half, or 513, made a grand total of 1,740 preferences. Over 

229 



2jo THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

one-half of this number expressed an interest in Social Psychology, the favorite 
division, while a little more than one-fifth evinced an interest in Statistical 
Sociology, which stood lowest in the selection. It would be hazardous to spec- 
ulate concerning the effect upon this distribution if the remaining members, 680, 
had also taken part in this informal referendum: 

Social psychology 279 

Teaching social science in the schools 249 

Social research 230 

Sociology and social work 230 

Community problems 200 

Educational sociology 159 

Rural sociology 141 

Biological factors in social causation 134 

Statistical sociology 118 

Total preferences as stated i , 740 

Activities of the Society 

The growth in the activities of the Society is shown by the fact that there 
were in 1924 representatives of our organization on five national bodies en aged 
in co-ordinating the work of social science associations or learned societies, and 
twelve standing and special committees of the Society. During the year the 
executive office sent out four communications from the President to the mem- 
bers of the Executive Committee and two communications to all the members 
of the Society, one from the President and the chairman of the Committee on 
Social Research, asking that research projects be submitted for consideration in 
the program to be devoted to research in progress, and the other from the chair- 
man of the Committee on Encyclopedia of the Social Sciences asking for data 
of help in organizing a plan. 

Respectfully submitted, 

ERNEST W. BURGESS, Secretary 



REPORT OP THE FINANCE COMMITTEE 

Your committee has, with the assistance of a public accountant, examined 
the books of the American Sociological Society for the fiscal year ending Novem- 
ber 30, 1924. The postings of the ledger have been checked and were found to be 
properly charged to the respective accounts. The balance in the bank as sub- 
mitted by the depository agrees with the statement as to "Cash in the Bank on 
November 30, 1924." The bonds of the Northwestern Electric Company and 
of the St. Cloud Public Service Company were examined and found satisfactory. 
We submit for your consideration Balance Sheet (Schedule "A") and Statement 
of Cash Receipts and Disbursements (Schedule "B") prepared by the Secretary- 
Treasurer on the basis of the report by the public accountant which was examined 
and found to be correct. 



REPORT OF THE FINANCE COMMITTEE 231 



BALANCE SHEET AS OF NOVEMBER 30, 1924 

Assets 
Cash in Bank ............... $ 383.64 

Office furniture ........... $118.65 

Less depreciation up to and including 1924 . . . 56.15 6 2.. 50 

Proceedings, on hand, 1,352 volumes at $0.50 ...... 676.00 

Investments: 

Northwestern Electric Co. 6 per cent Gold Bonds. . . . 500.00 

St. Cloud Public Service Co. 6 per cent Gold Bonds . . . 675. 38 



Surplus as of December i, 1923 ..... .... $2,481.34 

Additions: 

Increase in stock of Proceedings by 88 copies . $44.00 
Deductions: 

Depreciation Office Furniture . . $ 6.92 
Net Loss Schedule "B" .... 220.90 227.82 

Net deductions .............. 183.82 



Total liabilities $2,297.52 

SCHEDULE "B" 

STATEMENT OF CASH RECEIPTS AND DISBURSEMENTS FROM DECEMBER i, 1923, 
TO NOVEMBER 30, 1924 

Cash Receipts 

Dues from members, 1924 $3,822.40 

Dues from members, 1925 144.00 

Dues from life members 256.00 

$4,222.40 

Exchange with remittances 16.80 

Postage with remittances 2 . 85 

Income from Proceedings 326.49 

Interest on bonds 72.00 

Interest on certificate of deposit 26 . 24 

Receipts for abstract service 91.00 

Total receipts $4,757-78 

Plus credit from University of Chicago Press 350.00 

$5,107.78 

Cash Disbursements 

Proceedings, Volume XIX $1,444.00 

American Journal of Sociology 2,408.66 

Clerical aid, salaries, etc 511.03 

Postage and express 295 . 93 

Printing 290.69 

Stationary 147.90 

Secretary's expense at annual meeting 106.43 

Exchange on remittances 39 . 30 

Refund on memberships 50.31 

Auditing 10.00 

Insurance on Proceedings 2 . 50 

Office and miscellaneous expenses 21.93 

Total disbursements $5,328.68 

Excess of disbursements over receipts $ 220.90 



232 



THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 



Summary 

Balance in Bank, December i, 1923 $ 604.54 

Total receipts for period ending November 30, 1924 4,757.78 



Total disbursements for period ending November 30, 1924 . 
Less credit from University of Chicago Press .... 



$5,362.32 
$5,328-68 

350.00 $4,978.68 

Balance in bank, November 30, 1924 $ 383.64 

The Statement of Cash Receipts and Disbursements includes in its cash 
receipts "Dues from members, 1925" ($144.00). If these receipts had not been 
included, the balance "Cash in bank, November 30, 1924" ($383.64) would 
have been a balance of only $239.64. This balance, however, would have become 
a deficit if there were deducted from it the cash receipts from life members 
($256.00), income from the abstract service ($91.00), and the like figures from 
1923 against which no expenditures have been charged ($24.62), uninvested 
balance from life memberships, and $22.00 (subscriptions for abstract service). 
The deficit thus determined, $153.98, is, however, less than the deficit similarly 
computed for 1923 ($342.08). The difference in these two figures ($188.10) 
represents the second surplus in the accounts of the Society for seven years, and 
is largely to be attributed to the budget system which was introduced two years 
ago on the recommendation of the Finance Committee and to the effective work 
of the Editing Committee in bringing the cost of the Proceedings within the 
budget estimates. 

The Committee submits herewith a comparative table of incomes and ex- 
penditures for the last seven years, 1918-24 inclusive, prepared by the Treasurer. 

ANALYSIS OF ACTUAL INCOME AND EXPENDITURES 1917-1923 



Year 


Receipts from 
Dues 


Total Receipts 


Expenditures 


Deficit 


Cash Balance 


IOI7 










$180. 6s 


1018 


$2.411. 3.C 


$2,810. 70 


$2,863.87 


$^. 17 


327.48 


IOIO 


2 to8 3O 


2 062 7O 


7 . i n6 74 


27.2 QCf 


Q7 (T7 


IO2O 


3172 %O 


3CQI Q6 


3.81 s * oo 


272 QA 


T 70 41 


1021 . . . . 


7 . 708 tO 


4. 4OO . 7 3 


4..6l7. 22 


216.4.0 


346 oo 


IO22 . . 


4 228 72 


4..QO3. 70 


S.OO2. 7< 


08.06 


44 J 86 


y 1 
IQ23* 


4.43Q.4< 


?,OO7.86 


4,994.08 


103.78! 


342.08 


v ^ A 
1024. .... 


4722 4O 


<?, >l6.78 


<,128.68 


188 iof 


IC7. 08 















* The figures for 1923 and 1924 do not include receipts from life memberships nor for the abstract 
service. 

t Surplus. 

Your Committee respectfully recommends the consideration by the Execu- 
tive Committee of the following proposals: 

i. That an amendment to the Constitution be submitted to the Annual 
Meeting of the Society providing for a joint membership for husband and wife 
with annual dues of $5. The joint membership entitles the holder to a single 
subscription to the Journal and to one volume of the Proceedings. 



REPORT OF THE FINANCE COMMITTEE 



233 



2. That a spot map be made showing the geographical location of the 
members provided that the cost does not exceed $25. 

3. That a photograph of the President be included in the next and all 
succeeding volumes of the Proceedings. 

Your Committee begs leave to present herewith the third Annual Budget 
of the American Sociological Society covering the fiscal year ending November 
30, 1924. 

TENTATIVE BUDGET 

of the 

American Sociological Society for the Fiscal Year of 1925 
(December i, 1924 to November 30, 1925) 

Receipts 

Estimated 

Receipts for 

1925 

Dues from members $4,800.00 

Sale of publications 

Press credit 

Interest on bonds 

Interest on certificate of deposit .... 

Abstract service 

Exchange and postage with remittance and 
miscellaneous 



325-00 
350-00 

IOO.OO 

20.00 

100.00 

20.00 



Actual 


Actual 


Receipts for 


Receipts for 


1924 


1933 


$4,722.40 


$4,439.45 


326.49 


238.90 


350.00 


350.00 


72.00 


30.00 


26.24 


15.88 


107.00* 





19.65 



23.63 



Total receipts $5,?i5-oo $5,623.78 $5,097.86 



Expenditures 



American Journal of Sociology $2,400.00 

Proceedings 1,600. 

Clerical aid and salaries ' 

Postage and express 

Printing 

Stationary 

Secretary's expenses at meetings . 

Society membership A.C.L.S 

Auditing 

Exchange on dues 

Refunds on membership 

Insurance 

Abstract service 

Miscellaneous expense 

Total expenditures $5,715.00 $5,428.68 

^Receipts and estimated expenditures for abstract service included. 



Estimated 
Expenditures 
tor 1925 


Actual 
Expenditures 
for 1924 


$2,400.00 
I,6oo.oo 
600.00 


$2,408.66 
1,444.00 
5H.03 


300.00 
300.00 


295-93 
290 . 69 


150.00 
6o.OO 
60. oo 


147.90 
106.43 


10.00 


10.00 


40.00 


39.30 


55-00 


50.31 


3.00 

100.00 


2.50 
(est.) 100.00* 


37.00 


21.93 



Actual 

Expenditures 

tor 1923 

$2,302.80 

1,526.06 

432.85 

247.32 

189.45 

55-05 

18.43 

97.70 

IO.OO 

32.50 

54.70 

2.50 

24.72 
$4,994.08 



Respectfully submitted, 

Finance Committee: 
WILLIAM T. CROSS 
THOMAS D. ELIOT 
M. J. KARPF, Chairman 



234 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

ANNUAL REPORT or THE MANAGING EDITOR FOR THE 
FISCAL YEAR 

DECEMBER i, 1923 TO NOVEMBER 30, 1924 

On November 30, the number of different volumes of the Papers and 
Proceedings on hand was as follows: 

Volume Copies Volume Copies 

I 68 X 157 

II o Out of print XI o Out of print 

III o Out of print XII 80 

IV 36 XIII oOutofprint 

V 30 XIV 19 

VI o Out of print XV 256 

VII 31 XVI 153 

VIII 57 XVII 151 

DC 19 XVIII 305 

The total number of volumes, 1,352, is 88 more than were reported last 
year. 

Respectfully submitted, 

ERNEST W. BURGESS, Managing Editor 



THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

MINUTES OF THE EXECUTIVE COMMITTEE, CHICAGO, ILLINOIS, 
DECEMBER 29, 1924 

The meeting was called to order at 5 :io P.M. by President Charles A. Ellwood 
in Room 126, in the Auditorium Hotel. There were present, in addition to the 
President and the Secretary, Messrs. Blackmar, Cooley, Dealey, Galpin, Gillette, 
Gillin, Hayes, Lichtenberger, Ogburn, Park, Snedden, Weatherly, Willcox. 
(Only six of the twenty-one members of the Committee were absent.) The 
reading of the minutes of the last meetings was dispensed with, since they are 
printed in the Proceedings. 

President Ellwood stated that if there were no objections the first order of 
business would be the three proposals already submitted by correspondence to 
the members of the Committee. A motion by Professor Weatherly was passed 
that the President be authorized to appoint a Committee of three members to 
consult with representatives of other social sciences and through the Social 
Science Research Council upon the relation of the American Sociological Society 
to the American Association for the Advancement of Science, and to report its 
recommendation to the meeting of the Committee in 1925. 

A motion by Professor Blackmar carried providing that a committee of 
three persons be now appointed to consider the proposal of the election of 
distinguished foreign sociologists as honorary members of the Society. The pres- 
ident appointed U. G. Weatherly, E. C. Hayes, and J. Q. Dealey. 

After some discussion of the relation to the Society of groups and organiza- 
tions representing special interests, Professor Weatherly made a motion which 
carried that the President, Secretary, and one representative from each section 
within the Society and, by invitation, from organizations outside the Society, 
constitute an advisory committee to formulate a policy for the co-ordinating in 
the program both the general and special interests of members of the Society, 
provided that the policy recommended be referred to the Executive Commit- 
tee for action. 

Upon the report of a recommendation by the National Council for Social 
Studies that each of the social science associations defray the traveling expenses 
of its representative on the board of directors of the Council, Professor Park 
made a motion which carried that traveling expenses for this purpose to an 
amount not to exceed $50 be authorized. 

Professor Hayes presented a communication from Professor Consentini 
of the Internation Institute of Sociology and Social Politics of Turin suggesting 
affiliation with our Society. Professor Dealey's motion was passed referring this 

235 



236 TEE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

matter to the Committee to consider the election of distinguished foreign sociolo- 
gists to honorary membership. On motion of Professor Lichtenberger a similar 
request from Professor Marcel Mauss of the Institute of Sociology of France was 
referred to the Committee on Resolutions for an expression of felicitation upon 
the organization of the Institute and to the Committee on Honorary Member- 
ship of Foreign Sociologists for a report on the advisability of a plan of relation- 
ship. 

Moved and carried that telegrams of greeting to Professors Small and 
Giddings be sent by the Committee on Resolutions. 

On motion of Professor Ogburn the proposed amendment of the constitution 
of the Council of Learned Societies made necessary for the purpose of incorpora- 
tion was approved. 

A motion by Professor Lichtenberger passed that the Resolutions Com- 
mittee express the appreciation of the Society for the underwriting by the New 
York Times and Mr. Adolph S, Ochs of the authoritative work on American 
biography undertaken by the National Council of Learned Societies. 

The reports of the Secretary and of the Managing Editor for the year ending 
November 30, 1924, were read and approved. The report of the Finance Com- 
mittee was read and accepted. Professor Gillette moved the amendment to the 
constitution recommended by the Finance Committee providing for joint mem- 
bership in the Society of husband and wife with annual dues of five dollars be 
approved and referred to the annual business meeting for final action. The mo- 
tion was carried. 

A recommendation by Charles E. Merriam, chairman of the Social-Science 
Research Council was approved, that action on time and place of the next meet- 
ing of the Society be deferred in order to arrange for the selection of a common 
time and place for all the social-science associations. 

Respectfully submitted, 

ERNEST W. BURGESS, Secretary 



MINUTES OF THE BUSINESS MEETING FOR REPORTS OF COMMITTEES, 
CHICAGO, ILLINOIS, DECEMBER 30, 1924 

The meeting was called to order at 9 :io A.M. by President Charles A. Ellwood 
in the Banquet Hall, Auditorium Hotel. Reports, which are printed elsewhere 
in the Proceedings , were made by the chairmen of the following committees: 
the Committee on Social Research, W. F. Ogburn; the Committee on Social Ab- 
stracts, F. S. Chapin; the Committee on an Encyclopedia of the Social Sciences, 
H. B. Woolston; the Committee on the Classification of Civil Service Positions, 
J. P. Lichtenberger for Carl Kelsey, chairman. F. S. Chapin made a report of 
the work during the past year by the Social-Science Research Council, which is 
printed in full in this volume. 

Respectfully submitted, 

ERNEST W. BURGESS, Secretary 



MINUTES OF THE ANNUAL BUSINESS MEETING 237 

MINUTES OF THE ANNUAL BUSINESS MEETING, CHICAGO, ILLINOIS, 
DECEMBER 31, 1924 

The meeting was called to order at 9 : 05 A.M. by President Charles A. Ellwood 
in the Banquet Hall, the Auditorium Hotel. Since the minutes of the last meet- 
ing were printed in the Proceedings, their reading was dispensed with. The 
minutes of the meeting of the Executive Committee were read and approved. 
On the motion of Professor Gillette, the following amendment to the constitution 
submitted by the Executive Committee was adopted: 

That Article III on membership be amended to include the following section: A 
joint membership may be taken out by husband and wife upon the payment of an an- 
nual fee of five dollars. 

The committee appointed by the Executive Committee (U. G. Weatherly, 
E. C. Hayes, and J. Q. Dealey) to consider the proposal of the election of dis- 
tinguished foreign sociologists as honorary members of the Society, reporting 
through its chairman, made the following recommendations: 

1. That distinguished foreign sociologists may be elected to honorary membership 
in the Society. 

2. That a special committee of the Executive Committee be appointed to make 
nominations for each membership. 

3. That the Committee heartily concurs in the proposed plan of co-operation with 
such foreign sociological societies as have a recognized national standing, and such as 
contain among their membership men of international standing in the science. 

The Committee on Resolutions (A. J. Todd, E. S. Bogardus, and W. F. 
Ogburn) recommended the following resolutions which were adopted : 

1. The American Sociological Society expresses its deep appreciation to the Local 
Arrangements Committee, to the Chicago business men who provided the smoker 
Monday evening, to the management of the Auditorium Hotel for its many courtesies, 
to the City Club, the Cordon Club, Hull House, and other Chicago groups for their 
open-hearted hospitality and entertainment, all of which has contributed to the pleasure 
and success of our nineteenth annual meeting. 

2. The American Sociological Society in its nineteenth annual meeting assembled 
desires to express its enthusiastic appreciation of the munificence and rare wisdom dis- 
played by Mr. Adolph S. Ochs and the New York Times Company in underwriting to 
the extent of $500,000 the authoritative work on American biography projected by the 
American Council of Learned Societies. 

3. The American Sociological Society hereby records its belief that the time has 
now come when scholars from all countries should participate on equal terms in scien- 
tific congresses at which it is invited to be represented. The American Sociological So- 
ciety is of the opinion that no invitation to participate in a future international congress 
should be accepted by it unless the rules of the congress allow scholars of all countries 
to participate on equal terms. 

4. The American Sociological Society sends its greetings to, and offers fraternal 
good wishes for the success of, the newly organized Institut de Sociologie de France. 



238 THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 

The Committee on Nominations (E. C. Hayes, F. W. Blackmar, C. H. 
Cooley, and U. G. Weatherly) placed in nomination for the year 1925: president, 
Robert E. Park; first vice-president, John L. Gillin; second vice-president, 
Walter F. Willcox; secretary- treasurer, Ernest W. Burgess; members of the 
Executive Committee, Emory S. Bogardus, Howard W. Odum, and James E. 
Cutler. A motion made by Professor Gillette carried that the Secretary be 
instructed to cast the ballot of the Society for these nominees. Professor Hayes, 
as chairman of the Nominating Committee, raised the question of whether 
nominating committees in the future are required to follow the precedent of 
nominating to the presidency those who have held the offices of first and second 
vice-president. After some discussion a motion by Professor North carried that 
it is the sense of the Society that our present method of electing officers is un- 
satisfactory. A second motion by Professor North was then carried that it is 
the sense of the Society that nominating committees in the future submit at 
least two names for every office, without expressing a preference. President 
Ellwood then called President Park to the chair. Professor Hayes raised the 
question of the advisability of the re-election of presidents. A motion by Profes- 
sor Gillette carried that the practice of the renomination of presidents for a 
second term be not revived. 

Respectfully submitted, 

ERNEST W. BURGESS, Secretary 



AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 



MEMBERSHIP LIST FOR THE YEAR 1925 



ABBOTT, EDITH, University of Chicago, 

Chicago, 111. 
ABBOTT, W. LEWIS, 808 North Weber St., 

Colorado Springs, Colo. 
ABRAM, R. C., University of Missouri, 

Columbia, Mo. 
ABT, HENRY EDWARD, 6 Needham Place, 

Ithaca, N.Y. 
ADAMS, RALPH S., 46 Mt. Vernon St., 

Lansdale, Pa. 
ADAMS, SAMUEL B., 206 Gaston St., East, 

Savannah, Ga. 
ADDAMS, JANE, Hull House, 800 S. Hal- 

sted St., Chicago, 111. 
AKIN, MARJORIE E., Carle ton College, 

Northfield, Minn. 
ALBRIGHT, JOHN, Cayuga, Ind. 
ALBRIGHT, LEILA R., 1001 E. Jefferson 

Ave., Detroit, Mich. 
ALEXANDER, W. A., Library, Indiana 

University, Bloomington, Ind. 
ALEXANDER, W. M., Fayette, Mo. 
ALLPORT, FLOYD HENRY, University of 

Syracuse, Syracuse, N.Y. 

ALLPORT, GORDON W., Emerson Hall, 
Harvard University, Cambridge, 
Mass. 

ALMACK, JOHN C., Box 571, Stanford Uni- 
versity, Calif. 

ALTER, ADA M., 717 Ohio St., Lawrence, 
Kan. 

AMANN, DOROTHY, Library of Southern 
Methodist University, Dallas, Tex. 

ANDERSON, A. S., 220 S. sth Ave., La 
Grange, 111. 

ANDERSON, ROY R., Harle Ave., Cleve- 
land, Tenn. 

ANDERSON, WALFRED A., State College 
Station, Raleigh, N.C. 

ANDREWS, BENJAMIN R., i Old Wood 
Road, Edgewater, N.J. 

ANDREWS, JOHN B., American Associa- 
tion of Labor Legislation, 131 E. 
23d St., New York, N.Y. 

ANDREW, J. H., Storer College, Harpers 
Ferry, W.Va. 



ANGELL, ROBERT COOLEY, 2008 Day St., 
Ann Arbor, Mich. 

ANGLER, ROSWELL P., 140 Edgehill Road, 
New Haven, Conn. 

APPLEGATE, MELBOURNE S., 19 Grove 
Place, East Orange, N.J. 

D'ARDELL, VERDI D. S., 429 W. Gorham 
St., Madison, Wis. 

ARMSTRONG, ELSIE, Hayes Hotel, Chi- 
cago, 111. 

ARMSTRONG, SAMUEL TREAT, Hillbourne 
Farms, Katonah, N.Y. 

ARTMAN, J. M., University of Chicago, 
Chicago, 111. 

ASH, ISAAC E., Athens, Ohio 

ASHMAN, BENJAMIN II., Hoboken Beach, 
R.R. 4, Madison, Wis. 

ATHEY, MRS. C. N., 100 S. Patterson 
Park Ave., Baltimore, Md. 

AUBREY, EDWIN E., 407 S. Oak St., Ox- 
ford, Ohio 

AUSTIN, CHARLES B., 112 Cottage Ave., 
Mount Vernon, N.Y. 

AUSTIN, GERTRUDE B., 112 Cottage Ave., 
Mount Vernon, N.Y. 

AVERILL, EDITH S., Ivanhoe Road, Hil- 
grove, Cedar Rapids, Iowa 

AYERS, EDWARD EVERETT, 5553 Magnolia 
Ave., Lynchburg, Va. 

BABCOCK, DONALD C., University of New 
Hampshire, Durham, N.H. 

BABER, R. E., Blue Ridge, N. C. 

BABSON, ROGER, 5 Babson Park, Welles- 
ley Hills, 82, Mass. 

BADANES, SAUL, 32 Cameron Ave., Baby- 
lon, L.I., N.Y. 

BAILEY, ROY D., Morris County Chil- 
dren's Home, Boonton, N.J. 

BAIN, READ, University of Washington, 
Seattle, Wash. 

BAIRD, CHARLES GLENN, 330 Avenue A, 
Snohomish, Wash. 

BAKER, HERBERT M., Box 522, Greeley, 

Colo. 
BAKER, O. E., 1212 N. i6th St., Abilene, 

Tex. 



239 



240 



THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 



BAKER, OLIVER, i Hesketh St., Chevy 

Chase, Md. 
BAKKEN, H. H., 308 Oak St., Mount 

Horeb, Wis. 
BALCH, WILLIAM M., 610 N. 6th St., 

Baldwin City, Kan. 
BALDWIN, HELEN, Cascade, Iowa 
BALDWIN, SIMEON E., New Haven, Conn. 
BALES, RUTH, 5657 E. Washington St., 

Indianapolis, Ind. 
BALLARD, LLOYD VERNOR, 915 Park Ave., 

Beloit, Wis. 
BAMFORD, E. F., 1188 W. soth St., Los 

Angeles, Calif. 
BANNER, EDWARD A., 4609 N. Rockwell 

St., Chicago, 111. 
BARGER, J. WHEELER, Montana State 

College, Bozeman, Mont. 

BARNARD, MARGARET, 827 Bank St., 
Waterbury, Conn. 

BARNES, GERALD, 418 Thompson St., 
Ann Arbor, Mich. 

BARNES, HARRY E., 186 Elm St., North- 
ampton, Mass. 

BARNHART, KENNETH E., 1815 N. New- 
stod Ave., St. Louis, Mo. 

BARRY, A. GLEN, 416 N. Francis St., 
Madison, Wis. 

BARRY, Lois V., 10 Langdon St., Madi- 
' son, Wis. 

BATES, ROBERT L., Virginia Military In- 
stitute, Lexington, Va. 

BATTEN, S. Z., 1701 Chestnut St., Phila- 
delphia, Pa. 

BAUMG ARTEL, WALTER H., Larkspur, 

Colo. 
BAUMGARTEN, MRS. M. R., The Rosen 

baum Company, Pittsburgh, Pa. 

BEACH, WALTER G., Stanford University, 
Calif. 

BECKER, ETHEL, Gridley Hall, Carleton 
College, Northfield, Minn. 

BEDFORD, CAROLINE, 2221 Locust St., 
St. Louis, Mo. 

BEDFORD, SCOTT E. W., University of 
Chicago, Chicago, 111. 

BEELEY, ARTHUR L., University of Chi- 
cago, Chicago, 111. 

BEER, WILLIAM, Howard Memorial Li- 
brary, New Orleans, La. 

BELCHER, ALICE E., Milwaukee-Downer 
College, Milwaukee, Wis. 



BELLAMY, GEORGE A., 2723 Orange Ave., 
Cleveland, Ohio 

BELLAMY, RAYMOND, Florida State Col- 
lege for Women, Tallahassee, Fla. 

BELLER, WILLIAM F., 51 E. i23d St., New 
York, N.Y. 

BENGSTON, CAROLINE, Harvard, Nebr. 

BENJAMIN, PAUL L., 215 E. Walnut St'., 
Louisville, Ky. 

BENNIWIES, W. G., State Teachers' Col- 
lege, Greeley, Colo. 

BERMAN, WM. A., 711 Elwood Ave., N., 
Minneapolis, Minn. 

BERNARD, L. L., 13 Folwell Hall, Uni- 
versity of Minnesota, Minneapolis, 
Minn. 

BERNHEIMER, CHARLES S., 2612 Broad- 
way, New York, N.Y. 

BERNSTEIN, LUDWIG B., Federation of 
Jewish Charities, 502 Washington 
Trust Bldg., Pittsburgh, Pa. 

BERNSTEIN, WALTER, North Wales, 
Montgomery Co., Pa. 

BEST, HARRY, University of Kentucky, 
Lexington, Ky. 

BETTMAN, ALFRED, 1514 First National 
Bank Bldg., Cincinnati, Ohio 

BEVER, JAMES, 614 Ivy St., Bellingham, 
Wash. 

BICKHAM, MARTIN HAYES, 429 9th St., 
Wilmette, 111. 

BIDGOOD, LEE, Box 416, University, Ala. 

BINDER, RUDOLPH M., New York Uni- 
versity, Washington Square, New 
York, N.Y. 

BISHOP, EUGENE A., Maryland State 
Normal School, Towson, Md. 

BISHOP, H. MIRIAM, 360 Elm St., Elgin, 
111. 

BITTNER, C. J., 324 Fairchild St., Iowa 
City, Iowa 

BITTNER, W. S., 822 Hunter St., Bloom- 
ington, Ind. 

BIZZELL, WILLIAM B., A. & M. College, 

College Station, Tex. 
BLACHLY, MRS. MARGARET G. B., 241 

Willow Ave., Takoma Park, Md. 
BLACKMAR, FRANK W., University of 

Kansas, Lawrence, Kan. 
BLACKWELL, BESSIE T., Wellsville, Mo. 
BLAGDEN, EDWARD S., 113 E. 64th St., 

New York, N.Y. 
BLAINE. MRS. EMMONS, 101 E. Erie St., 

Chicago, 111. 



MEMBERSHIP LIST 



241 



BLANCHARD, DR. PHYLLIS, Child Guid- 
ance Clinic, Children's Hospital of 
Philadelphia, i8th and Bainbridge 
Sts., Philadelphia, Pa. 

BLANZET, ERNEST M., 9i6-5th St., S.E., 
Minneapolis, Minn. 

BLAZIER, GEORGE J., Marietta College, 
Marietta, Ohio 

BLUMER, HERBERT E., 1108 Paquin St., 
Columbia, Mo. 

BLUMER, MILDRED G., Green, Iowa 

BODENHAFER, WALTER B., Washington 
University, St. Louis, Mo. 

BOETTIGER, Louis A., Y.M.C.A., Apple- 
ton, Wis. 

BOGARDUS, EMORY S., University of 
Southern California, Los Angeles, 
Calif. 

BOHANON, ALONZA, Atlanta University, 
Atlanta, Ga. 

BOHN, DR. FRANK, 135 Prospect St., 
Stamford, Conn. 

BOND, NATHANIEL BATSON, Mississippi 
Woman's College, Hattiesburg, Miss. 

BONDY, ROBERT E., American Red Cross, 
National Headquarters, Washing- 
ton, D.C. 

BONNEY, ETHELIND M., 1213 S. California 
Ave., Stockton, Calif. 

BOODIN, JOHN ELOF, 717 E. 2d St., 
Northfield, Minn. 

BOOKMAN, C. M., Council of Social Agen- 
cies, 25 E. pth St., Cincinnati, Ohio 

BOOKSTABER, PHILIP D., Harrisburg, Pa. 

BOORMAN, W. RYLAND, Y.M.C.A. Col- 
lege, 5315 Drexel Ave., Chicago, 111. 

BOOTHE, VIVA B., 301 Irvine Place, El- 
mira, N.Y. 

BORETZ, MARY E., 438 E. 87th St., New 
York, N.Y. 

BORNETT, MRS. MARY CLARKE, 5812 
Maeburn Rd., Pittsburgh, Pa. 

BOSSARD, JAMES H. S., Logan Hall, Uni- 
versity of Pennsylvania, Phila- 
delphia, Pa. 

BOSTICK, ARTHUR E., St. Louis Public 
Library, St. Louis, Mo. 

BOUCKE, O. FRED, School of Liberal Arts, 
State College, Pa. 

BOWDEN, A. O., Silver City, N.M. 

BOWDEN, R. D., Box 206, Glen Ellyn, 111. 

BOWEN, R. H., The Parker. Hanover, 
N.H. 



BOWERMAN, GEORGE F., Public Library, 

Washington, D.C. 

BOWMAN, C. A., Albright College, Myers- 
town, Pa. 
BOWMAN, LEROY E., 593 Kent Hall, 

Columbia University, New York, 

N.Y. 
BOYD, NEVA L., 800 S. Halsted St., 

Chicago, 111. 
BOYER, EDWARD S., 4949 Indiana Ave. 

Chicago, 111. 
BOYSON, BELLE D., 774 Piedmont Ave., 

N., Atlanta, Ga. 
BRADBURN, HELEN WATSON, 2412 Cal- 

houn St., New Orleans, La. 
BRADEN, SAMUEL R., Carlinville, 111. 
BRADLEY, REV. D WIGHT J., Ministers 

Study, Webster Grove, Mo. 
BRAMER, JOHN PHILIP, 95-28th St., 

Elmhurst, L.I., N.Y. 
BRANDENBURG, S. J., Clark University, 

Worcester, Mass. 
BRANSON, DR. E. C., University of North 

Carolina, Chapel Hill, N.C. 
BREAKER, JASPER, Atlanta University, 

Atlanta, Ga. 
BRECKINRIDGE, SOPHONISBA P., Green 

Hall, University of Chicago, Chicago, 

111. 
BRENNAN, Miss A. F., 688 Boylston St., 

Boston, Mass. 
BRIDGES, THOMAS A., 858 Albany St., 

Boston, Mass. 

BRISTOL, Lucius MOODY, University of 
Florida, Gainesville, Fla. 

BROGAN, A. P., University of Texas, 
Austin, Tex. 

BROOK, MARGARET M., 1305-8111 Ave., 
Worthington, Minn. 

BROOKE, MYRTLE, Montevallo, Ala. 

BROOKS, ELIZABETH, 622 W. State St., 
Jacksonville, 111. 

BROOKS, LEE M., 69 Montvale St., Ros- 
lindale, Boston, Mass. 

BROOME, EDWIN W., Rockville, Md. 
BROWN, HARRIET G., Elmira College, 

Elmira, N.Y. 
BROWN, LAWRENCE G., 804 E. 58th St., 

Chicago, 111. 
BROWN, THOMAS I., Morgan College, 

Baltimore, Md. 
BROWNE, SQUIRE F., University of Idaho, 

Moscow, Idaho 



242 



THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 



BRUNNER, EDMUND DE S., Mountain BYRNES, AGNES H. M., 279 Lexington 



Lakes, NJ. 
BRUNO, FRANK J., 404 S. 8th St., Minne- 
apolis, Minn. 
BRUNS, JOHN, 245 E. 75th St., New York, 

N.Y. 
BUCHAN, EVELYN, University of Maine, 

Orono, Me. 
BUCKLEY, DANIEL, Woodrow, Broad Axe, 

Ambler, Pa. 
BUDD, DORIS MARY, 1032 N. Prospect St., 

Tacoma, Wash. 
BUELL, LEONARD WATSON, 559 Main St., 

Stoneham, Mass. 
BULLOCK, CHARLES E., Canton, Pa. 

BURDETTE, MRS. ROBERT J., Hotel 

Maryland, Pasadena. Calif. 
BURGER, W. H., 2 W. 45th St., New York, 
N.Y. 



Ave., Bryn Mawr Club, New York, 

N.Y. 
BYRON, WILLIAM F., Hull House, 800 S. 

Halsted St., Chicago, 111. 
CADWALLADER, STARR, 2901 Weymouth 

Rd., Cleveland, Ohio 
CALHOUN, ARTHUR W., Brookwood, 

Katonah, N.Y. 
CAMPBELL, MARGARET G., 2616 E. 7$d 

St., Chicago, 111. 

CAMPBELL, W ALTER J., Y.M.C.A. Col- 
lege, Springfield, Mass. 
CANIS, EDWARD N., R. A, Box 372-A, 

Indianapolis, Ind. 
CAPEN, EDWARD W., 146 Sargent St., 

Hartford, Conn. 
CAPONE, AMERICO ARTHUR, 343 Vale St., 

Chelsea, Mass. 



BURGESS, E. W., University of Chicago, CARNEY MABEL, Teachers' College, Col- 



Chicago, 111. 
BURKE, W. W., University of Chicago, 
Chicago, 111. 



umbia University, New York, N.Y. 
CAROTHERS, MARIE ANNIS, in N. Indi- 
ana Ave., Bloomington, Ind. 



BURKHARD, SAMUEL, 926 Forest Ave., CARPENTER, ALLAN R., c/o Boy Scouts 



Tempe, Ariz. 

BURLESON, F. E., A.R.C., 1709 Washing- 
ton Ave., St. Louis, Mo. 

BURNET, PHILIP, Continental Life Insur- 
ance Company, Wilmington, Dela. 

BURNHAM, E. LEWIS, Berwyn, Pa. 

BURNHAM, ERNEST, 1532 Grand Ave., 
Kalamazoo, Mich. 

BURR, WALTER, Kansas State Agricul- 
tural College, Manhattan, Kan. 

BURT, HENRY J., Hendrix College, Con- 
way, Ark. 

BURTON, ERNEST R., 450 Milton Road, 
Rye, N.Y. 



of Ameria, 7 S. Crawford Ave., Chi- 
cago, 111. 
CARPENTER, NILES, 196 Lisbon Ave., 

Buffalo, N.Y. 
CARR, G. E., Librarian, International 

Y.M.C.A., Springfield, Mass. 
CARR, LOWELL J., c/o Henry Brown, 

Washington, Mich. 
CARROLL, JOSEPH C., 400 Grove St., 

Farmvfile, Va. 
CARROLL, MOLLIE RAY, Goucher College, 

Baltimore, Md. 
CARSTENS, C. C., Child Welfare League of 

America, 130 E. 22d St., New York, 

N.Y. 



BUSCH, HENRY MILLER, Union Theologi- CARTER, HUGH S., Wharton School, Uni- 



cal Seminary, 3041 Broadway, New 
York, N.Y. 
BUSHEE, FREDERICK A., 1207 Aurora 
Ave., Boulder, Colo. 



versity of Pennsylvania, Philadel- 
phia, Pa. 

CARVER, MEREL, 143 Butler Ave., Indian- 
apolis, Ind. 



BUSHNELL, C. J., 702 The Majestic, 1802 CARVER, THOMAS N., 7 Kirkland Ave., 



Cherry St., Toledo, Ohio 



BUTLER, Clara W., 519 W. i23d St., New CASE 
York, N.Y. 



Cambridge, Mass. 



BUTTENHEIM, HAROLD S., American City 



E, CLARENCE M., University of 
Southern California, Los Angeles, 



Calif. 



Magazine, 443~4th Ave., New York, CASE, MILLS E., 24 Broad St., New York 



N.Y. 



N.Y. 



BUTTERFIELD, KENYON L., East Lansing, CASIS, LILIA M., University of Texas, 



Mich. 



Austin, Tex. 



BYRNE, MARY GERTRUDE, 1523 Foucher CASON, HULSEY, Syracuse University, 



St., New Orleans, La. 



Syracuse, N.Y. 



MEMBERSHIP LIST 



243 



CASS, GEORGE WILLARD, 96 St. James St., 

Mansfield, Pa. 
CASWELL, OMAR, Tarkio College, Tarkio, 

Mo. 
CATTELL, J. McKEEN, Garrison-on-Hud- 

son, N.Y. 
CAVAN, JORDAN, Rockford College, 

Rockford, 111. 
CAVANAGH, GLADYS, 2223 Chamberlaine 

Ave., Madison, Wis. 
CHAFFEE, JS^RS. GRACE E., 643 Grant St., 

Iowa City, Iowa 
CHAMBERLAIN, JOSEPH P. 510 Kent Hall, 

Columbia University, New York, 

N.Y. 

CHAMBERLIN, VELL BURROWS, 611 Em- 
met St., Ypsilanti, Mich. 
CHANDLER, G. L., Mt. Vernon, Iowa 
CHANEY, LUCIAN W., Department of 

Labor, Washington, D.C. 
CHAPIN, F. STUART, Folwell Hall 17, 

University of Minnesota, Minneap- 
olis, Minn. 
CHAPLAIN, VIOLA L. F., 515 N. 7th St., 

Richmond, Va. 
CHASE, TEREE S., American Mission, 

Teheran, Persia 
CHASSELL, JOSEPH OLIN, 3041 Broadway, 

New York, N.Y. 
CHATTER JEE, MANMATHA NATH, 119 

Davis St., Yellow Springs, Ohio 
CHU, T. S., Conservancy Engineering 

College, Nanking, China 
CLARK, EMERSON WILLIAMS, Box 285, 

Athens, Ohio 
CLARK, VIVIAN, 2115 Penn Ave., S., 

Minneapolis, Minn. 
CLARK, PEARL E., 223 W. J St., Ontario, 

Calif. 

CLARK, ROBERT FRY, Marietta College, 

Marietta, Ohio 
CLARK, WILLIS W., 4259 S. Van Buren 

Place, Los Angeles, Calif. 
CLARKE, EDWIN L., 606 i3th Ave., S.E., 

Minneapolis, Minn. 
CLARKE, HELEN L, Public Welfare 

Association, ^ 22 N. Hancock St., 

Madison, Wis. 

CLINE, DENZEL C., University of Wash- 
ington, Seattle, Wash. 
CLOW, FREDERICK R., Oshkosh, Wis. 
Co ATE, LOWELL H., 1500 Atchison St., 

Pasadena, Calif. 
COE, ARTHUR W., Glenwood, Minn. 



COEN, B. F., Fort Collins, Colo. 

COLBECK, MYRON B., 402 i9th Ave., E., 
Superior, Wis. 

COLE, MRS. FREDERICK H., 1810 Spencer 
St., Omaha, Neb. 

COLE, WILLIAM L, Wheaton College, Nor- 
ton, Mass. 

CONARD, P. A., Mercedes 1071, Monte- 
video, Uruguay, S.A. 

CONBOY, IRENE KATHRYN, 2243 Julian 
St., Denver, Colo. 

CONDON, MARY L. R., Margaret Morrison 
Division, Carnegie Institute of Tech- 
nology, Pittsburgh, Pa. 

CONKLIN, RUTH G., 5534 S. Marshfield 
Ave., Chicago, 111. 

CONNER, JOHN V., Box 582, Catholic 
University, Washington, D.C. 

CONRAD, F. A., University Station, Tuc- 
son, Ariz. 

CONRAD, MRS. IRENE FARNHAM, 1517 
Broadway, New Orleans, La. 

COOK, DR. E. ALBERT, R. 2, Chickasha, 
Okla. 

COOLEY, CHARLES H., 703 Forest Ave., 
Ann Arbor, Mich. 

COOLIDGE, MRS. DANE, Mills College, 
Calif. 

COOPER, JOHN M., Caldwell Hall, Catho- 
lic University, Washington, D.C. 

CORNELIUS, E. T., Apartado 78, Aguas 
Calientes, Mexico 

CORWIN, GRACE HUNTINGTON, Burkley 
Place, Iowa City, Iowa 

COULTER, CHAS. W., Ohio Wesley an Uni- 
versity, Delaware, Ohio 

COWPER, MRS. MARY 0., 1017 Dacian 
Ave., Durham, N.C. 

Cox, E. M., Montfort, Wis. 

CRAVENER, GEORGE M., 1238 Phila- 
delphia St., Indiana, Pa. 

CRESSEY, PAUL Go ALB Y, 5757 University 
Ave., Chicago, 111. 

CROMWELL, MARY E., 1815-13^1 St., 
N.W., Washington, D.C. 

CROOKS, EZRA B., Newark, Del. 

CROSS, WILLIAM T., 5748 Kenwood Ave., 
Chicago, IU. 

CROSSAN, BERTHA J., 3801 Walnut St., 
Philadelphia, Pa. 

GROSSMAN, LYMAN T., 9113 Commercial 
Ave., Chicago, 111. 

CULPEPPER, MARIAN, 1202 E. 34th St., 
Kansas City, Mo. 



244 



THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 



CUMMINGS, ARTHUR, Indiana State Nor- 
mal School, Terre Haute, Ind. 

CUMMINGS, F. L., Shorewood, Milwaukee, 
Wis. 

CUMMINS, E. E., 206 Nassau St., Prince- 
ton, N J. 

CUMMINS, Nora B., State Normal School, 
Bellingham, Wash. 

CUTLER, J. E., Western Reserve Uni- 
versity, Cleveland, Ohio 

DAILEY, Miss DEW, Parkersburg, Iowa 

DALKE, DEIDERICH L., Cameron, Mo. 

DANIEL, J. W. W., Wesleyan College, 
Macon, Ga. 

DANIEL, V. E., Wiley University, Mar- 
shall, Tex. 

DANIELSON, MERLE G., 1444 K St., 
Lincoln, Neb. 

DAUCHY, MRS. SAMUEL, 525 Hawthorne 
Place, Chicago, 111. 

DAVIE, MAURICE R., Yale University, 
New Haven, Conn. 

DAVIES, ANNA F., 433 Christian Street, 
Philadelphia, Pa. 

DAVIES, G. R., University of North Da- 
kota, Grand Forks, N.D. 

DAVIES, STANLEY P., Room 710, 105 E. 
22d St., New York, N.Y. 

DAVIS, BERENICE D., 5045 Drexel Blvd., 
Chicago, 111. 

DAVIS, EDWARD H., P.O. Drawer 1217, 
Waterbury, Conn. 

DAVIS, JEROME, mo Edwards Hall, Yale 
University, New Haven, Conn. 

DAVIS, MICHAEL M., JR., 20 S. Broadway, 
White Plains, N.Y. 

DAWBER, M. A., Board of Home Mis- 
sions, Philadelphia, Pa. 

DAWSON, C. A., Montreal, Quebec, Can. 

DAWSON, FLOYD W., 1724 Madison St., 
Madison, Wis. 

DAY, GRACE A., Board of Education 
Offices, Meriden, Conn. 

DEALEY, G. B., The "Dallas News", Dal- 
las, Tex. 

DEALEY, JAMES Q., Brown University, 
Providence, R.I. 

DEAN, JESSIE E., 4912 Marathon St., 
Los Angeles, Calif . 

DEARDORFP, NEVA, Bryn Mawr College, 
Bryn Mawr, Pa. 

DEDRICK, CALVERT L., 2512 McKinley 
St., Madison, Wis. 



DEENEY, CORNELIUS F., University of 
Santa Clara, Santa Clara, Calif. 

DEBTS, LEE EMERSON, Apartment 2, 
3089 Broadway, New York, N.Y. 

DE GRAFF, HARMON O., 5757 University 
Ave., Chicago, 111. 

DEIBLER, FREDERICK SHIPP, 2119 Sher- 
man Ave., Evans ton, 111. 

DEICH, VALENTINE, L. B. 897, Gooding, 
Idaho 

DENNISON, HENRY S., Dennison Manu- 
facturing Co., Framingham, Mass. 

DENUNE, PERRY P., 240 E. Maynerd 
Ave., Columbus, Ohio 

DETWEILER, FREDERICK G., Denison 
University, Granville, Ohio 

DE VAULT, G. G., University Club, Madi- 
son, Wis. 

DEWALD, BERTHA, 22 Morris St., New 
Brunswick, N.J. 

DEWEY, F. A., 22 William St., New York, 

N.Y. 

DE WOLF, GAIL, Currier Hall, University 
of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa 

DEXTER, ROBERT C., 103 Spring St., 
Saratoga Springs, N.Y. 

DICKERSON, ROY E., 1 201 Federal Re- 
serve Bank Bldg., Kansas City, Mo. 

DlEFENDERFER, PAUL TlLTON, 5523 

Dorchester Ave., Chicago, 111. 

DlEFFENDORFER, R. E., 150 Fifth Ave., 

New York, N.Y. 

DIETRICH, ETHEL B., Mount Holyoke 
College, South Hadley, Mass. 

DILLMAN, WILLIS LsRoY, 448 Monas- 
tery Ave., Philadelphia, Pa. 

DINGLEDINE, RAYMOND C., 821 S. Main 
St., Harrisonburg, Va. 

DISERENS, CHARLES MURDOCH, Univer- 
sity of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, Ohio 

DITTMER, C. G., Sterling Hall, Madison, 
Wis. 

DODGE, DOROTHY, 15 S. Gilman St., 
Madison, Wis. 

DOE, HELEN, Dover, N.H. 

DORN, REV., GEORGE, 2602 Farnam St., 
Omaha, Neb. 

DOUGE, ROBERT, Apartment 44, 2412 
Seventh Ave., New York, N.Y. 

Dow, G. S., University of Denver, Den- 
ver, Colo. 

DOWD, JEROME, University of Oklahoma, 
Norman, Okla. 



MEMBERSHIP LIST 



245 



DRACHSLER, JULIUS, 413 W. i47th St., 
New York, N.Y. 

DREIS, ED. J., Filton, Santa Cruz Co., 
Calif. 

Du Bois, CHARLES G., 195 Broadway, 
New York, N.Y. 

DUFLOT, J. J., Canyon, Tex. 

DUKE, SAMUEL P., State Teachers Col- 
lege, Harrisonburg, Va. 

DUMMER, Mrs. W. F,, 679 N. Michigan 
Ave., Chicago, 111. 

DUNCAN, H.* G., 3551 University Ave., 
Los Angeles, Calif. 

DUTHIE, MARY EVA, 101 Eddy St., Ithaca, 
N.Y. 

EARL, ANNA E., University of Minnesota, 
Minneapolis, Minn. 

EARP, F. L., Drew Theological Seminary, 
Madison, N.J. 

EAVES, LUCILE, 109 Davis Ave., Brook- 
line, Mass. 

EBY, S. C., 307 Amsterdam Ave., Man- 
hattan, N.Y. 

EDDY, SARAH J., Bristol Ferry, R.I. 

EDWARDS, LYFORD P., St. Stephens Col- 
lege, Annandale-on-Hudson, N.Y. 

EELLS, H. L., 22 E, Court St., Iowa City, 
Iowa 

EGARTNER, Z. T., Adams and Scoville 
Sts., Oak Park, 111. 

ELDRIDGE, SEBA, University of Kansas, 
Lawrence, Kan. 

ELIOT, THOMAS D., Northwestern Uni- 
versity, Evanston, 111. 

ELIOT, WILLIAM G., Ill, 1724 I St., 
Washington, D.C. 

ELKUS, ABRAM I., 956 Madison Ave., 
New York, N.Y. 

ELLIOTT, PAUL CLINTON, 2000 S. Figueroa 
St., Los Angeles, Calif. 

ELLIS, GENEVIEVE, 314 S. Mills St., 
Madison, Wis. 

ELLWOOD, CHARLES A., 407 College Ave., 
Columbia, Mo. 

ELMER, M. C., University of Minnesota, 
Minneapolis, Minn. 

ELSTON, JAMES S., Travelers Insurance 
Co., Hartford, Conn. 

EMIG, ARTHUR SAMUEL, Alleghany Col- 
lege, Meadville, Pa. 

EUBANK, E. E., 835 Blair Ave., Cincin- 
nati, Ohio 

EVELAND, MARGARET GERTRUDE, 107 S. 
Buchanan St., Maryville, Mo. 



EVERETT, M. S., 68 Middle Divinity Hall, 

University of Chicago, Chicago, 111. 
EWING, H. E , Paseo Colon 161, Buenos 

Aires, Argentina, S. A. 
EWING, JOHN B., Ottawa University, 

Ottawa, Kan. 
EWING, KATHARINE, 1261 ist Ave., E, 

Cedar Rapids, Iowa 
EZEKIEL, JEANNETTE, 2101-1 6th St., 

N.W., Washington, D.C. 

FAIRBANK, MAY, Cornell College, Mount 
Vernon, Iowa 

FAIRCHILD, H. P., 185 St. Ronan St., 
New Haven, Conn. 

FAIRCHILD, MILTON, Chevy Chase, Wash- 
ington, D.C. 

FARIS, ELLSWORTH, University of Chi- 
cago, Chicago, 111. 

FARNUM, H. W., 43 Hillhouse Ave., New 

Haven, Conn. 
FARRA, KATHRYN, 130 E. 22d St., New 

York, N.Y. 

FARRINGTON, WENDELL FREMONT, Liver- 
more Falls, Me. 
FARROW, TEERA, Box 369, New Haven, 

Conn. 

FAUST, CHARLES J., Rogers, N.D. 
FEHLANDT, A. F., 917 Watson St., Ripon, 

Wis. 
FELTON, RALPH A., 205 Fairmount Ave., 

Ithaca, N.Y. 
FERRIS, EMILY H., 1126 Tennessee St., 

Lawrence, Kan. 

FERRIS, HON. WOODBRIDGE W., Big Rap- 
ids, Mich. 

FEUCHTWANGER, JOSEPH, JR., 127 W. 
Gilman St., Madison, Wis. 

FEUERJLICHT, MORRIS M., 3034 Wash- 
ington Blvd., Indianapoh's, Ind. 

FIESER, JAMES L., Apt. 207, 3945 Con- 
necticut Ave., Washington, D.C. 

FINNEY, Ross L., College of Education, 
University of Minnesota, Minneap- 
olis, Minn. 

FISHER, GALEN M., 39 Grand View Ave., 
Pleasantville, N.Y. 

FISHER, IRVING, Yale University, New 
Haven, Conn. 

FISHTINE, EDITH, 35 Glenarm St., Dor- 
chester, Mass. 

FISKE, ESTLE, 502 N. Quincy St., Indian- 
apolis, Ind. 

FISKE, G. WALTER, Oberlin College, Ober- 
lin, Ohio 



246 



THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 



FLANAGAN, THOMAS J., 21 Larkin St., 

Atlanta, Ga. 
FLEMING, HERBERT E., 206 Fairview 

Road, Glencoe, 111. 
FLORA, CHARLES P., 155 Church St., 

Watertown, Mass. 
FLYNN, ELEANOR J., 1102 W. Johnson St., 

Madison, Wis. 

FOLEY, ROY WILLIAM, Colgate Univer- 
sity, Hamilton, N.Y. 
FOLKS, GERTRUDE, 25 E. pth St., Cin- 
cinnati, Ohio 
FOLLETT, MARY P., 5 Otis Place, Boston, 

Mass. 
FOLLMER, HAROLD N., 9 High St., Selins- 

grove, Pa. 

FOLSOM, JOSEPH KIRK, Sweet Briar, Va. 
FOOTE, W. W., Library, State College of 

Washington, Pullman, Wash. 
FORD, MRS. RICHARD T., Sagamore Hotel, 

Rochester, N.Y. 
FOSTER, EMERY M., 315 Channing St., 

N.E., Washington, D.C. 
FOSTER, ROBERT G., Nevada Extension 

Service, College of Agriculture, Reno, 

Nev. 
FOSTER, SOLOMON, 90 Treacy Ave., 

Newark, NJ. 

Fox, MRS. ANNA B., 264 Sumner St., 
Buffalo, N.Y. 

FRANK, JACOB Louis, 1254 S. Troy St., 
Chicago, 111. 

FRANK, ROBERT NORTH, McCormick 
Theological Seminary, Chicago, 111. 

FRANKEL, EDWARD T., 501 W. i3$th St., 
New York, N.Y. 

FRAZIER, EDWARD FRANKLIN, Morehouse 
College, Atlanta, Ga. 

FREDEN, GUSTAV, 815 Bloomington St., 
Iowa City, Iowa 

FRENCH, A. N., Durham, N.H. 

FRENCH, HENRY S., 1701 Arch St., Phila- 
delphia, Pa. 

FREY, FRED C., Louisiana State Univer- 
sity, Baton Rouge, La. 

FREY, OSCAR N., P.O. Box 307, Smyrna, 
Del. 

FRIEDMAN, ELISHA M., 2275 83d St., 
Brooklyn, N.Y. 

FRIEDMAN, H. G., 202 Riverside Drive, 
New York, N.Y. 

FRY, C. LUTHER, 370 7th Ave., New York, 
N.Y. 



FRYSINGER, Miss GRACE E., 2400 i6th 
St., N.W., Washington, D.C. 

FUGH, PAUL C., 301 Dryden Road, 
Ithaca, N.Y. 

FULLER, FREDERIC HENRY, 277 Brook 
St., Providence, R.I. 

GAISER, JOSEPH, Billings Polytechnic, 
Billings, Mont. 

GALPIN, C. J., Department of Agricul- 
ture, Bureau ( of Agricultural Eco- 
nomics, Washington, D.C. 

GANSEN, EDWARD, 114 Otter St., Osh- 
kosh, Wis. 

GARDNER, C. S., 2407 Longest St., Louis- 
ville, Ky. 

GARDNER, MRS. MARY A. MACHIN, Bart- 
lett Hall, Ripon College, Ripon, Wis. 

GARNER, EVELYN B., 7628 Essex Ave., 
Chicago, 111. 

GARNETT, W. E., A. & M. College, College 
Station, Tex. 

GARST, DR. JULIUS, 29 Oread St Worces- 
ter, Mass. jff 

GARVIN, MARY B., 400 Wt/n8th St., 
New York, N.Y. 

GAR WOOD, L. E., Coe College, Cedar 
Rapids, Iowa 

GARY, FRANK E. H., 10 Tremont St., 
Boston, Mass. 

GAVIN, HELENA, 1 128 E. 62d St., Chicago, 
111. 

GEER, CURTIS M., 155 Broad St., Hart- 
ford, Conn. 

GEHLKE, C. E., 671 E. io8th St., Cleve- 
land, Ohio 

GEOGHEGAN, J. HERBERT, 316 $th St., 
Hoquiam, Wash. 

GEORGE, JULIA, 1136 Eddy St., San 
Francisco, Calif. 

GERLACH, TALITHA, 3455 Guilford Ave., 
Indianapolis, Ind. 

GETTYS, W. E., McGill University, Mon- 
treal, Quebec, Can. 

GIDDINGS, FRANKLIN H., Columbia Uni- 
versity, New York, N.Y. 

GIESE, DR. CHARLES OSCAR, Ferguson 
Bldg., Colorado Springs, Colo. 

GIFFORD, WILLIAM A., 756 University St., 

Montreal, Can. 
GILBERT, ERNEST DE WITT, 4 College 

Place, Defiance, Ohio 
GILBERT, WILLIAM E., 616 Tyler Ave., 

East Radford, Va. 



MEMBERSHIP LIST 



247 



GILBERT, WILLIAM M., Drew Theo- 
logical Seminary, Madison, N.J. 

GILCHRIST, OLIVE B., 41 Linnaean St., 
Cambridge, Mass. 

GILFILLAN, S. C., 1 1 20 Summer St., 
Grinnell, Iowa 

GILLETTE, JOHN M., North Dakota Uni- 
versity, Grand Forks, N.D. 

GILLIN, J. L., Sterling Hall, University of 
Wisconsin, Madison, Wis. 

GILMAN, CHARLOTTE P., 380 Washington 
St., Norwich Town, Conn. 

GILTNER, EMMETT E., 212 W. i2oth St., 
New York, N.Y. 

GLENN, JOHN M., 130 E. 22d St., New 
York, N.Y. 

GLUCKSMAN, H. L., 807 Riverside Drive, 
New York, N.Y. 

GODARD, GEORGE S., State Library, 
Hartford, Conn. 

GOETHE, C. M., 720 Capitol National 
Bank Bldg., Sacramento, Calif. 

GOETSCH, E. W., 22 E. Court St., Iowa 
City, Iowa 

GOLDMAN, JACOB, 1444 Vine St., Lincoln, 
Neb. 

GOLDSMITH, EMMA PEARL, Hampton In- 
stitute, Hampton, Va. 

GOLDSMITH, SAMUEL A., Bureau Jewish 
Social Research, 114 Fifth Ave., 
New York, N.Y. 

GOLDSTEIN, DR. JULIUS, Beckstrasse 87, 
Darmstadt, Germany 

GOOD, ALVIN, State Normal School, 
Natchitoches, La. 

GOODCHILD, SUSAN E., n Lemon St., 
Providence, R.I. 

GOODMAN, E. URNER, 916 Walnut St., 
Philadelphia, Pa. 

GOODNOW, JEAN, 1019 Ottawa St., Lans- 
ing. Mich. 

GOODNOW, NELLE THOMPSON, New Jersey 
Sanatorium, Glen Gardner, N.J. 

GORDON, WILLIAM RALPH, 124 Pugh St., 
State College, Pa. 

GOTTSCHALL, ROBERT J., IO2I W. 

Marshall St., Norristown, Pa. 
GOULD, KENNETH M., 1158 Murrayhill 
Ave., Pittsburgh, Pa. 

GRADY, REV. JOSEPH E., 70 Frank St., 

Rochester, N.Y. 
GRAHAM, ARCHIE ALLEN, Box 177, 

Phoebus, Va. 



GRAHAM, VIRGINIA C., Atlanta Univer- 
sity, Atlanta, Ga. 

CRANBERRY, JOHN C., Southwestern Uni- 
versity, Georgetown, Tex. 

GRAVES, MAUD C., 2223 E. 93d St., Chi- 
cago, 111. 

GRAY, R. S., Commonwealth Club, 345 
Sutter St., San Francisco, Calif. 

GRAY, WARD M., Boys' Division 
Y.M.C.A., Central Branch, Chicago, 

GREELEY, EDITH, 5428 Woodlawn Ave., 
Chicago, 111. 

GREEN, ELEANOR B., 14 John St., Provi- 
dence, R.I. 

GREEN, LORAINE RICHARDSON, 5528 
Ingleside Ave., Chicago, 111. 

GREENWOOD, MILDRED P., 221 Piedmont 
Ave., Atlanta, Ga. 

GRIEST, LOUISE, 29th and Indiana, To- 
peka, Kan. 

GRIFFITH, PEARL, 37 Winsor Ave., 
Watertown, Mass. 

GRIMM, ALBERTA, R.E. Box 59, Indian- 
apolis, Ind. 

GROOM, EDITH, Warren, 111. 

GROSSMANN, Louis, 3311 E. Ocean Blvd., 
Long Beach, Calif. 

GROTE, CAROLINE, Monroe Hall, Ma- 
comb, 111. 

GROVE, ELSA BUTLER, 43 West St., 
Northampton, Mass. 

GROVER, CHESTER A., Grover Block, 
Evanston, 111. 

GROVES, ERNEST R., 74 Warren St., 
Needham, 92, Mass. 

GUILD, JUNE PURCELL, 2354 Monroe St., 
No. 7, Toledo, Ohio 

GURINIAN, V. G., 801 Washington St., 
Oak Park, 111. 

HAAS, REV. FRANCIS J., St. Francis Semi- 
nary, St. Francis, Wis. 

HACKBUSCH, FLORENTINE, 112 Market 
St., Harrisburg, Pa. 

HAHNE, ERNEST H., 6028 Kenwood Ave., 
Chicago, 111. 

HAJICEK, STANLEY T., 6417 Kenwood 
Ave., Chicago, 111. 

HALE, MABEL F., c/o H. H. Hale, 6 
Beacon St., Boston, Mass. 

HALL, A. B., University of Wisconsin, 
Madison, Wis. 

HALL, O. F., 829 Salisbury St., West La- 
fayette, Ind. 



248 



TEE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 



HALL, ROYAL GLENN, 5452 Kimbark 

Ave., Chicago, III. 
HALLEY, Lois KATE, 108 S. 7th St., 

Columbus, Miss. 
HALSEY, R. V., 2101 Washtenaw Ave., 

Ann Arbor, Mich. 
HAMBLIN, JOSEPHINE, 15 Pond St., Hyde 

Park, Boston, Mass. 
HAMILTON, ELEANOR B., 137 Langdon St., 

Madison, Wis. 
HAMILTON, JAMES A., Library, Roberts 

College, Constantinople, Turkey 
HAMMOND, KATHARINE, c/o Mrs. Curtis, 

Huntsville, Tex. 
HANCOCK, GORDON BLAINE, Virginia 

Union University, Richmond, Va. 
HANDMAN, M. S., 1305$ Rio Grande St., 

Austin, Tex. 
HANKINS, FRANK H., Smith College, 

Northampton, Mass. 
HARING, DOUGLAS GILBERT, c/o C. B. 

Tenny, 229 Simai Cho, Ushigome, 

Tokyo, Japan 
HARPER, ERNEST B., 331 Douglas Place, 

Kalamazoo, Mich. 
HARPER, J. C., Prospect and Torrey, La 

Jolla, Calif. 

HARPER, M. L., Memphis, Tenn. 

HARRIMAN, EDWARD A., 735 Southern 
Bldg., Washington, D.C. 

HARRIS, ELLEN W., 233 Langdon St., 
Madison, Wis. 

HARRIS, I. G., M.D., Brooklyn State 
Hospital, Brooklyn, N.Y. 

HARRIS, MILTON H., Utah Agricultural 
College, Logan, Utah 

HARRIS, THOMAS L., West Virginia Uni- 
versity, Morgantown, W.Va. 

HARRISON, REV. EDWARD, Niagara Uni- 
versity, Niagara, N.Y. 

HARRISON, SHELBY M., Russell Sage 

Foundation, 130 E. 22d St., New 

York, N.Y. 
HART, CLYDE W., i Bella Vista Place, 

Iowa City, Iowa 
HART, HASTINGS H., Russell Sage 

Foundation, 130 E. 22d St., New 

York, N.Y. 

HART, HORNELL, Bryn Mawr, Pa. 
HARTING, CAROLINE E. E., 418 S. gth St., 

St. Joseph, Mo. 
HARTMAN, GEORGE EDGAR, Box 752, 

State College, Montgomery, W.Va. 



HARTMANN, MARY C., 5631 Dorchester 

Ave., Chicago, 111. 
HARVEY, BEATRICE, Box 966, San Juan, 

Porto Rico 

HARWARD, GEORGE N., Newton, Ala. 
HATCHER, J. WESLEY, 196 i3th Ave., 

Columbus, Ohio 
HAWTHORNE, H. B., 425 Welch Ave., 

Ames, Iowa 
HAYES, C. WALKER, 5725 Kenwood Ave., 

Chicago, 111. 
HAYES, E. C., 808 W. Oregon'St., Urbana, 

111. 
HAYES, ELIZABETH M., 701 University 

Ave., Minneapolis, Minn. 
HAYES', ROBERT B., George R. Smith 

College, Sedalia, Mo. 
HAYNER, NORMAN S., University of Wash- 
ington, Seattle, Wash. 
HAYNES, FRED EMORY, State University, 

Iowa City, Iowa 
HEARD, MRS. D WIGHT B., Casa Blanca, 

Phoenix, Ariz. 
HEBBARD, CHARLES, Davenport Hotel, 

Spokane, Wash. 
HEESMANCE, EDGAR LAING, 241 Lawrence 

St., New Haven, Conn. 
HEPFNER, W. C., Thiel College, Green- 
ville, Pa. 
HEINMILLER, H. W., no Columbia Ave., 

Naperville, 111. 

HEISTER, A. V., Franklin and Marshall 
College, Lancaster, Pa. 

HEITMAN, EMILY, 1928 S. Harvard Blvd., 
Los Angeles, Calif. 

HELLEBERG, VICTOR EMANUEL, 1725 
Mississippi Ave., Lawrence, Kan. 

HENDRICKSON, H. O., 404 E. Monroe St., 
Mt. Pleasant, Iowa 

HENKE, FREDERICK G., 643 William St., 

Meadville, Pa. 
HERRON, STELLA, 1933 Elysian Fields, 

New Orleans, La. 

HERTZLER, C. W., 164 E. Center St., 
Berea, Ohio 

HERTZLER, JOYCE O., Social Science Hall, 
University of Nebraska, Lincoln, 
Neb. 

HESS, GEORGE O., Oelwein High School, 

Oelwein, Iowa 
HEWES, AMY, Mount Holyoke College, 

South Hadley, Mass. 
HEWITT, THERON, Burnham, Pa. 



MEMBERSHIP LIST 



249 



HEXTER, MAURICE B., 25 Tremont St., HOLT, ARTHUR E., 5757 University Ave., 



Boston, Mass. 



Chicago, 111. 



HICKEY, JOSEPH A., Villanova College, HOLT, COL. L. T., West Point, N.Y. 



Villanova, Pa. 
HICKMAN, THOMAS S., 20 Beacon St., 

Boston, Mass. 
HICKS, MARY L., Public Health Federa- 



HOOVER, H. D., Carthage College, Carth- 



age, 111. 
HOPKINS, Louis J., 
St. Augustine, ] 



Shenandoah St., 



tion, 25 E. 9th St., Cincinnati, Ohio HORAK, JACOB, 1700 Avoca Ave., Du- 



HILL, B. M., 688 Boylston St., Boston, 

Mi 



HILL, ROBERT T., Education Bldg., Toledo, Ohio 



buque, Iowa 
HORN, FRANKLIN L., 1342 Prospect Ave., 



Albany,'N.Y. 



HOTCHKISS, MARY A., 65 Ridge St., 



Organizing Family Social Work, HOUGHTON, LEROY K., 25 Richmond St., 



130 E. 22d St., New York, N.Y. 



Vrrx J-V-H-N , J^jfiiJ\\J J. J-V., ^^ J 

West Roxbury, Mass. 



KILLER, E. T., 314 Lincoln Hall, Urbana, HOUSE, FLOYD N., University of Chicago, 



111. 



Chicago, 111. 



HILLHOUSE, MRS. LEWIS 1542 St. Leger HOUSE, J. T., State Teachers College, 
Place, Cincinnati, Ohio J -' - 6 ' 

HIMES, NORMAN EDWIN, 25 Hammond 
St., Cambridge, Mass. 

HITCHCOCK, J. E., Oberlin, Ohio 

HOBEN, ALLAN, Kalamazoo College, 



Kalamazoo, Mich. 
HODAPP, ALOYS PHILIP, St. Thomas Col- 



Wayne, Neb. 
HOVERSTAD, T. A., 2312 Alden St., St. 

Paul, Minn. 
HOWARD, BERTHA J., University of 

Maine, Orono, Me. 



HOWARD, CLARA EULALIE, 214 Pope St., 
Benton, 111. 



lege, St. Paul, Minn. 

HODGSON, CASPAR W., Yonkers-on- 
Hudson, N.Y. 

HOERNER, HENRY, Columbus College, 
Sioux Falls, S.D. 

HOFFER, C. R., State College, East Lans- 
ing, Mich. 

HOFFER, FRANK N., University of North How E CLOICE R -> T - c - u -> Fort Worth > 
Carolina, Chapel Hill N. C. 

HOFFMAN, CHARLES W., 417 Warren 
Ave., Cincinnati, Ohio 



HOWARD, GEORGE E., 815 S. 35th St., 
Lincoln, Neb. 

HOWARD, MAJOR T. J., Gammon Theo- 
logical Seminary, Atlanta, Ga. 

How AT, WILLIAM F., 832 Hohman St., 
Hammond, Ind. 



Tex. 



HOWELL, SARAH A., 73 Van Ness Place, 
Newark, N.J. 

HOFFSOMMER, HAROLD CHARLES, Agri- HowERTH, I. W., State Teachers College, 



cultural College, N.D. 



Greeley, Colo. 



HOLBEN, RALPH P., Dartmouth College, Hu, Ti CHIEN, 5647 Drexel Ave., Chicago, 



Hanover, N.H. 



111. 



HOLBROOK, DAVID H., 130 E. 22d St., HUDSON, W. M., 420 E. Anderson St., 



New York, N.Y. 
HOLLAND, WILLIAM W., 216 Calhoun St., 
Cincinnati, Ohio 



Greencastle, Ind. 
HUGH, DR. Yu TINN, Higher Normal 
College, Peking, China 



HOLLINGSHEAD, GEORGE G., 47 Afterglow HUGHES, EVERETT C., 563$ University 



Way, Montclair, N.J. 



Ave., Chicago, 111. 



HOLLOWAY, WILLIAM, Talladega College, HUGHES, GWENDOLYN S., 370 7th Ave., 



Talladega, Ala. 



New York, N.Y. 



HOLMES ? JOHANNA, 4044 Ruckle St., HUMMEL, B. L., 9 Kuhlman Court, 

Indianapolis, Ind. Columbia, Mo. 

HOLMES, MERRILL JACOB, 1219 W. Uni- HUNT, ELIZABETH PINNEY, Walnut Lane, 

versity Blvd., Mitchell, S.D. Bryn Mawr, Pa. 

HOLMES, ROY H., 602 E. Liberty St., HUNTINGTON, GERTRUDE M., 303 S, 

Ann Arbor, Mich. Chestnut St., Platteville, Wis. 



250 



TEE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 



HURSH, E. M., Westerville, Ohio 
HUSTED, JEANETTE, 907 Wright St., 

Champaign, 111. 
HUTCHINSON, C. R., 1438 E. 6oth St., 

Chicago, 111. 
HUTCHISON, F. L., American Institute of 

Electrical Engineering, 33 W. 39th 

St., New York, N.Y. 
HYDE, D. CLARK, Faculty of Economics, 

Keio University, Mita, Tokyo, Japan 
HYPES, J. L., Connecticut Agricultural 

College, Storrs, Conn. 
ILES, R. EDGAR, P.O. Box 188, Langston, 

Okla. 

INGERSOLL, RAYMOND V., 149 S. Oxford 

St., Brooklyn, N.Y. 
IRSHAY, Z., Westville, 111. 
IRVIN, DORIS E., 107 E. 34th St., New 

York, N.Y. 
IRVING, BERTHA A., 102 Henderson Ave., 

New Brighton, S.I., N.Y. 

ISRAEL, HENRY, 1849 Grand Central 
Terminal Bldg., New York, N.Y. 

IWEN, WILBERT P., Pine Island, Minn. 

JACOBS, THEO, 226 Somersett Road, 
Roland Park, Baltimore, Md. 

JAMES, MRS. MAY HALL, 22 Weston Ave., 
Arlington, Cranston, R.I. 

JAMGOCHIAN, SAMUEL H., Lafayette Col- 
lege, Easton, Pa. 

JARRETT, MARY C., 16 Carver St., Bos- 
ton, Mass. 

JEDDELOH, HENRY J., 356 W. i22d St., 
New York, N.Y. 

JEFFERS, JOHN, 507 Genesee Ave., Sagi- 

naw, Mich. 
JENKINS, W. S., Benton County Abstract 

and Title Co., Prosser, Wash. 
JENKS, ALBERT E., National Research 

Council, Washington, D.C. 
JENSEN, HOWARD E., Butler College, 

Indianapolis, Ind. 
JEREMIAH, J. A., 117 W. s8th St., New 

York, N.Y. 
JESSUP, STEVE M., 518 N. Van Buren 

St., Iowa City, Iowa. 

Jo, Miss FUMI, Maita, Yokohama, Japan 
JOCHER, KATHERINE C., 228 E. Franklin 

St., Chapel Hill, N.C. 
JOHANSEN, JOHN PETER, Y.M.C.A., 

i3th and P Sts., Lincoln, Neb. 
JOHNSON, CHARLES D., Baylor Univer- 
sity, Waco, Tex. 



JOHNSON, CLARENCE R., Bucknell Uni- 
versity, Lewisburg, Pa. 

JOHNSON, F. E., Federal Council of 
Churches, 105 E. 22d St., New York, 
N.Y. 

JOHNSON, FRANKLIN, 358 Harvard St., 
Cambridge, Mass. 

JOHNSON, FRED R., 1180 Lawrence Ave., 
Detroit, Mich. 

JOHNSON, GLENN R., 310 McCluer St., 
Greensboro, N.C. 

JOHNSON, GUY B., Box 652, Chapel Hill, 
N.C. 

JOHNSON, HARRIETT E., 32 Chestnut St., 
Boston, Mass. 

JOHNSON, NOBEL GUSTAV, 632 S. Wash- 
ington St., Paxton, 111. 

JOHNSTON, MARY, The Community Com- 
mittee, 25 E. 3oth St., New York, 
N.Y. 

JONAS, RALPH, 115 N. Broadway, New 
York, N.Y. 

JONES, LESTER M., Morningside College, 
Sioux City, Iowa 

JONES, ROBERT ELIJAH, Box 103, Wave- 
land, Miss. 

JONES, THOMAS JESSE, 101 Park Ave., 
New York, N.Y. 

JONES, WILLIAM H., West Virginia Col- 
legiate Institute, Institute, W.Va. 

JOSEPH, ISAAC, Box 690, Cleveland, Ohio 

JOSEPH, MAURICE, 25 E. 9th St., Cin- 
cinnati, Ohio 

JOSLYN, CARL G., 33 Bowdoin St., Cam- 
bridge, Mass. 

Jo WE, S. Y., c/o Lung E, Han, Hiungao, 
Fengtien, China 

KAHN, EDWARD M., Jewish Peoples Insti- 
tute, 1258 W. Taylor St., Chicago, 

KARICKHOFF, EARLE O., West Virginia 
Wesleyan College, Buckhannon. 
W.Va. 

KARPF, MRS. FAY B., 210 W. gist St., 
New York, N.Y. 

KARPF, M. J., 210 W. 9ist St., New York, 

KARSTINS, CHARLES E M Zion Church 
Rectory, Dobs Ferry-on-Hudson, 
N.Y. 

KATO, MASUO, c/o Mitsuma, 60 Shim- 
machi, Yoyogi, Tokyo, Japan 

KAWABE, KISABURO, 483 Nakano-Koenji, 
Tokyo, Japan 



MEMBERSHIP LIST 



251 



KAWAMURA, TADAO, 5757 University 
Ave., Chicago, 111. 

KEATOR, A. D., University of North 
Dakota, University Station, Grand 
Forks, N.D. 

KELLER, A. G., Room 226, S.L.H., Yale 
Station, New Haven, Conn. 

KELLOGG, MRS. F. R., 25 Colles Ave., 
Morristown, N.J. 

KELSEY, CARL, University of Pennsyl- 
vania, Philadelphia, Pa. 

KENNEDY, ELMER, 1934 S. Union St., 
Chicago, 111. 

KENNEDY, FRANCIS W., Heidelburg Uni- 
versity, Tiffin, Ohio 

KENWORTHY, DR. MARION E., Bureau of 
Children's Guidance, 9 W. 48th St., 
New York, N.Y. 

KEPECS, JACOB, Levindate, Station E, 
Baltimore, Md. 

KERBY, WILLIAM J., Catholic University, 
Washington, D.C. 

KERN, R. R., Washington University, 
2023 G St., N.W., Washington, D.C. 

KERPAN, JOSEPH LUKE, 83 Chittenden 
Ave., Columbus, Ohio 

KETCIIAM, DOROTHY, Social Service De- 
partment, University of Michigan, 
Ann Arbor, Mich. 

KEY, DR. WILHELMINE, Sanatarium, 
Battle Creek, Mich. 

KIEFFER, GEORGE LINN, Rosedale, N.Y. 

KILPATRICK, EARL, Extension Division of 
University of Oregon, 650-52 Court 
House, Portland, Ore. 

KIMBALL, REGINALD STEVENS, State 
Normal School, Worcester, Mass. 

KIMMELL, ELSIE M., R.R.I., Box 59, 
Kewanee, 111. 

KINCHELOE, SAMUEL C., Y.M.C.A. Col- 
lege, 5315 Drexel Ave., Chicago, 111. 

KING, MRS. EDITH SHATTO, 69 W. nth 
St., New York, N.Y. 

KING, JUDSON, 637 Munsey Bldg., Wash- 
ington, D.C. 

KING, WILLIS J., Gammon Theological 
Seminary, Atlanta, Ga. 

KINGSBURY, SUSAN M., Roberts Road 
and Montgomery Ave., Bryn Mawr, 
Pa. 

KINGSLEY, NATHAN G., Thomas A. Doyle 
School, Providence, R.I. 

KINNEMAN, JOHN A., State Normal 
School, West Chester, Pa. 



KLRK, WILLIAM, Pomona College, Clare- 

mont, Calif. 
KIRKLAND, ISABELLE, 79 Chandler St., 

Boston, Mass. 
KIRKPATRICK, CLIFFORD, 865 Main St., 

North Leominster, Mass. 
KIRKPATRICK, E. L., Division of Farm 

Population and Rural Life, Bureau 

of Agriculture and Economics, 

U.S.D.A., Washington, D.C. 
KIXMILLER, HELEN G., Texas Woman's 

College, Fort Worth, Tex. 
KLEE, MAX, 1340 E. 48th St., Chicago, 

111. 

KLEIHEGE, GEORGE W., Hoisington, Kan. 
KLEIN, PHILIP, American Association of 

Social Workers, 130 E. 22d St., 

New York, N.Y. 
KNEBEL, LEWIS, 2947 S. Lyndale Ave., 

Minneapolis, Minn. 
KNEELAND, HILDEGARDE, Bureau of 

Home Economics, U.S. Department 

of Agriculture, Washington, D.C. 
KNICKERBOCKER, REV. H. W., 2411 

Whitney St., Houston, Tex. 
KNIGHT, M. M., Barnard College, Colum- 
bia University, New York, N.Y. 
KOCH, W. T., Northwestern University, 

Evanston, 111. 
KOHL, CLAYTON C., Normal College, 

Bowling Green, Ohio 
KOLB, J. H., College of Agriculture, Madi- 
son, Wis. 
KOTHMEIER, EMMA K., Whiting Hall, 

Knox College, Galesburg, 111. 
KRAKOWER, ISADORE E., 367 Vermont 

St., Brooklyn, N.Y. 
KREIGER, HERBERT W., U.S. National 

Museum, Division of Ethnology, 

Washington, D.C. 

KREBS, O. A., 619 Clymer Ave., Madison, 

Wis. 
KROUT, MAURICE H., 420 S. Kildare Ave., 

Chicago, 111. 
KRUEGER, E. T., Vanderbilt University, 

Nashville, Tenn. 
KUBLY, LOUISE S., R.F.D. 7, Monroe, 

Wis. 
KUHLMAN, A. F., University of Missouri, 

Columbia, Mo. 
KULP, DANIEL H., II, Teachers College, 

Columbia University, New York, 

N.Y. 
KUNNECKE, F. J., University of Dayton, 

Dayton, Ohio 



252 



THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 



KURSHEEDT, ALPHONSE H., 12 W. 44th 

St., New York, N.Y. 
KUSAMA, SHIKO, De"le*gation du Japan, 

Socie*t6 des Nations, 9 Rue La Perou, 

Paris, France 
KYES, D. H., 828 Gilby Ave., St. Paul, 

Minn. 
LANDERS, J. S., Oregon Normal School, 

Monmouth, Ore. 
LANDESCO, JOHN, 6026 Ellis Ave., 

Chicago, 111. 
LANDIS, BENSON Y., Federal Council of 

Churches, 105 E. 22d St., New York, 

N.Y. 

LANG, GEORGE, University, Ala. 
LANGMAN, HARRY, 846 Prospect Place, 

Buffalo, N.Y. 
LANTIS, L. O., State Normal School, 

Minot, N.D. 
LATHROP, JULIA C., 1204 National Ave., 

Rockford, 111. 
LATTIMORE, ELEANOR LARABEE, 

Y.W.C.A., 59 E. Monroe St., Chi- 

cago, 111. 
LATJDER, FRANK, 707 Long Bldg., Kansas 

City, Mo. 
LAUGHLIN, S. B., 1605 Court St., Salem, 

Ore. 
LAUNE, FERRIS F., Wieboldt Foundation, 

3166 Lincoln Ave., Chicago, 111. 
LAWLESS, REV. ALFRED, JR., 154! Auburn 

Ave., Atlanta, Ga. 
LAWSON, VICTOR F., 1500 Lake Shore 

Drive, Chicago, 111. 
LEE, FREDERIC EDWARD, n Marion St., 

Hyattsville, Md. 

LEFAVOUR, HENRY, 119 Bay State Road, 
Boston, Mass. 

LE FEVRE, MAUD, 1332 K St., Lincoln, 

Neb. 

LEH, L. L., Page, Neb. 
LEHMAN, E. E., 2-1 5th Ave., Columbus, 

Ohio 
LEHMAN, W. C., 43 Broadway, Hacken- 

sack, N. J. 
LEIPZIGER, EMIL WILLIAM, 1708 Dufossat 

St., New Orleans, La. 
LE MOYNE, MRS. FRANCIS J., Green 

Spring Ave., Roland Park P.O., 

Baltimore, Md. 
LENICHECK, FRANK A., 650 28th St., 

Milwaukee, Wis. 
LESEM, JOSEPHINE, 1539 Rosemont Ave., 

Chicago, 111. 



LEVEY, BEATRICE Z., 102 E. Oak St., 
Chicago, 111. 

LEVINGER, LEE J., 1722 Hancock St., 
Wilmington, Del. 

LEWIS, E. ST. ELMO, 2170 E. Jefferson 
Ave., Detroit, Mich. 

LICHTENBERGER, J. F., Logan Hall, Uni- 
versity of Pennsylvania, Phila- 
delphia, Pa. 

LILLIEFORS, MANFRED, JR., 621 W. Elm 
St., Chicago, 111. 

LIMBERG, WAYNE, Plymouth, Wis. 

LIND, ANDREW W., 1116 Eastlake Ave., 
Seattle, Wash. 

LINDEMAN, E. C., High Bridge, NJ. 

LINDENBERGER, L. H., 129 Raymond 
Ave., Louisville, Ky. 

LINDSAY, SAMUEL M., Columbia Uni- 
versity, New York, N.Y. 

LINDSEY, EDWARD, National Bank Bldg., 
Warren, Pa. 

LIVELY, CHARLES E., Department of 
Rural Economics, Ohio State Uni- 
versity, Columbus, Ohio 

LIVELY. E. L., State Normal School, 
Fairmont, W.Va. 

LLOYD, ALFRED H., 1735 Washtenaw 
Ave., Ann Arbor, Mich. 

LOBER, MABEL, 410 Spring Mill Ave., 
Conshohocken, Pa. 

LOEBER, MAUD, New Medical Bldg., New 
Orleans, La. 

LOFSTEDT, CHRISTINE, 233 Columbia St., 
Pasadena, Calif. 

LOGAN, H. A., Randolph-Macon Woman's 
College, Lynchburg, Va. 

LONDOW, EZEKIEL JACOB, Box 198, New 
Jersey Federation, Newark, N.J. 

LONG, THOMAS ALEXANDER, Johnson C. 
Smith University, Charlotte, N.C. 

LONGACRE, LINDSAY B., 2273 S. Filhnore 
St., Denver, Colo. 

LONGAKER, FRANK C., Box 625, Hickory, 
N.C. 

LOOMIS, FRANK D., 1340, 10 S. LaSalle 
St., Chicago, 111. 

LORING, REV. ROBERT SPRAGUE, P.O. 
Box 694, Milwaukee, Wis. 

LOTHROP, MARGARET M., Stanford Uni- 
versity, Calif. 

LOVEJOY, OWEN R., National Child 
Labor Committee, 215 Fourth Ave., 
Room 1702, New York, N.Y. 
LOWDEN, HON. FRANK O., Oregon, 111. 



MEMBERSHIP LIST 



253 



LOWE, MARGARET S., Box 62, Neillsville, 

Wis. 
LOWMAN, RUTH M., 805 Peoples Bank 

Bldg., McKeesport, Pa. 
LUCAS, FRANCES S., 1032 Beacon Ave,, 

Los Angeles, Calif. 
LUCEY, REV. ROBERT EMMETT, 825 

Higgins Bldg., Los Angeles, 

Calif. 
LUEHRING, F. W., 709 7th St., S.E., 

Minneapolis, Minn. 

LUM, CHARLES M., 786 Broad St., New- 
ark, NJ. 
LUMLEY, F. E., Page Hall, Ohio State 

University, Columbus, Ohio 
LUNDBERG, EMMA O., Apt. 703, The 

Woodward, Washington, D.C. 
LUNDBERG, GEORGE A., University of 

Washington, Seattle, Wash. 
LUNDEEN, EARNEST W., Bethany, Neb. 

LUPTON, FLORENCE, 3240 Kenwood Ave., 
Indianapolis, Ind. 

LURIE, HARRY L., 1800 Selden St., Chi- 
cago, 111. 

LYDAY, JUNE F., Psychopathic Hospital, 
Iowa City, Iowa 

MCBRIDE, ANNA CHRISTINE, Depart- 
ment of Social Work, Carnegie Insti- 
tute of Technology, Pittsburgh, 
Pa. 

McCAULEY, HELEN MARIE, 1219 Elm- 
wood Ave., Evanston, 111. 

MCCLENAHAN, BESSIE A., 705 Missouri 
Ave., Columbia, Mo. 

McCLUER, FRANC L., Westminster Col- 
lege, Fulton, Mo. 

McCLURG, RUTH M., 1631 Monroe St., 

Madison, Wis. 
McCoY, BRUCE R., 1217 W. Dayton St., 

Madison, Wis. 
MCDONALD, LOUISE, 625 N. Temple Ave., 

Indianapolis, Ind. 
McDouGLE, IVAN E., Goucher College, 

Baltimore, Md. 
MACFARLAND, CHARLES S., Room 612, 

105 E. 22d St., New York, N.Y. 
McGiBBON, LEONA M., 1620 Oaks Ave., 

Superior, Wis. 
MCGREGOR, TRACY W., 1453 Brush St., 

Detroit, Mich. 
McKEE, J. T., State Normal School, 

Florence, Ala. 
McKiBBEN, LEWIS, 7036 Union Ave., 

Chicago, 111, 



MCKINLOCK, GEORGE A., 316 S. Wells St., 

Chicago, 111. 
MACLEOD, WILLIAM CHRISTIE, 2558 N. 

29th St., Philadelphia, Pa. 
MCMAHON, THERESA, 4026 loth St.. 

N.E., Seattle, Wash. 
MACMILLAN, J. W., Victoria College, 

Toronto, Ontario, Can. 
MACPHERSON, HECTOR, Corvallis, Oregon 
MCKIBBEN, LEWIS L., 333 Transylvania 

Park, Lexington, Ky. 
MCKENZIE, R. D., University of Wash- 
ington, Seattle, Wash. 
MACLEAN, ANNIE MARION, 257 E. Belle- 

vue Drive, Pasadena, Calif. 
McLEAN, LEE D., Alleghany College, 

Meadville, Pa. 
MCWILLIAMS, ROBERT HUGH, 951 S. 9th 

St., Salina, Kan. 
MACARAIG, SERAFIN E., University of the 

Philippines, Manila, P.I. 
MACKEY, MYRA P., 11131 S. Irving Ave., 

Chicago, 111. 
MACURDA, ARTHUR A., 1609 W. 9th St., 

Los Angeles, Calif. 
MACY, V. EVERIT, 24 Broad St., New 

York, N.Y. 
MADDOX, BRADY P., 517 N. Chicago St., 

Joliet, 111. 

MAEL, JOHN, R.F.D. Madison, Wis. 
MAGUIRE, REV. JOHN W. R., St. Victor 

College, Kankakee, 111. 
MALONE, R. E., Agricultural, Mechanics 

and Normal School, Pine Bluff, Ark. 
MANGOLD, GEORGE B., 4002 Lexington 

Ave., St. Louis, Mo. 
MANN, ALBERT Z., Hamline University, 

St. Paul, Minn. 
MANN, DELBERT M., 733 Missouri St., 

Lawrence, Kan. 

MANN, ROWENA MORSE, 5653 Dor- 
chester Ave., Chicago, III. 
MANNY, THEODORE B., Hendrix College, 

Conway, Ark. 
MAPHIS, OMER B., 3435 W. Van Buren 

St., Chicago, 111. 
MARGOLD, CHARLES W., 1384 Carrol St., 

Brooklyn, N.Y. 
MARK, MARY LOUISE, 64 W. Woodruff 

Ave., Columbus, Ohio 
MARKELL, HELEN CHANDLER, 5616 El- 
liott St., Philadelphia, Pa. 
MARKEY, JOHN F. ; 1101 7th St., S.E,, 

Mineapolis, Minn, 



254 



THE AMERICAS SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 



MARKS, RALPH M., South High School, 
Omaha, Neb. 

MARKS, J. WYATT, University of Okla- 
homa, Norman, Okla. 

MARSHALL, AGNES M., 541 Eastern Park- 
way, Brooklyn, N.Y. 

MARSHALL, L. C., 1320 E. s6th St., Chi- 
cago, 111. 

MARSTON, PHILIP MASON, Box 471, 
Durham, N.H. 

MARTIN, DR. EVERETT DEAN, 150 W. 
nth St., New York, N.Y. 

MARTIN, ISAAC V., Medicine Lodge, Kan. 

MARTIN, PAUL E., P.O. Box 204, Beloit, 
Wis. 

MASSEY, W. F., Barneveld, Wis. 

MATHER, SAMUEL, Western Reserve 
Bldg., Cleveland, Ohio 

MATHES, LENA B., 1200 Security Bldg., 
Chicago, 111. 

MATSUMOTO, RYOZO, 9 Lathrop St., 
Madison, Wis. 

MATTESON, ANGELA B., 822 Clymer 
Place, Madison, Wis. 

MATTHEWS, HAROLD J., Superintendent 
of Public Welfare, Jefferson City, Mo. 

MAUTNER, BERTRAM H., Colorado Col- 
lege, Colorado Springs, Colo. 

MAUZY, FRANK, Potomac State School, 
Keyser, W.Va. 

MAXSON, C. H., Bishop College, Marshall, 
Tex. 

MAYER, JOSEPH, Tufts College, Mass. 

MAYNE, JOHN R., 314 E. Leigh St., Rich- 
mond, Va. 

MECKLIN, JOHN M., Dartmouth College, 
Hanover, N.H. 

MEHUS, O. MYKING, 3812 29th Ave., S., 
Minneapolis, Minn. 

MEIER, NORMAN C., 211 Liberal Arts 
Bldg., University of Iowa, Iowa City, 
Iowa 

MELCHIOR, MARY ELIZABETH, State 
Normal School, West Chester, Pa. 

MELOY, LUELLA P., Pennsylvania Col- 
lege for Women, Pittsburgh, Pa. 

MELVIN, BRUCE L., State College of Agri- 
culture, Cornell University, Ithaca, 
N.Y. 

MERCER, ISABEL, 515 N. Lake St., Madi- 
son, Wis. 

MERONEY, W. P., Baylor University, 
Waco, Tex. 



METCALP, HENRY C., 61 Paine Ave., New 
Rochelle, N.Y. 

MEYER, ADOLF, Johns Hopkins Univer- 
sity, Baltimore, Md. 

MEYERS, MRS. H. H., 3390 San Marino 
St., Los Angeles, Calif, 

MEZ, DR. JOHN, University of Arizona, 
Tucson, Ariz. 

MILLER, ARTHUR L., 316 Huntington 
Ave., Boston 17, Mass. 

MILLER, B. E., 306 Hicks Ave., Columbia, 
Mo. 

MILLER, H. A., 1517 Perry Ave., Colum- 
bia, Ohio 

MILNE, MAUDE, Rosholt, S.D. 

MINER, ORA, Southern Methodist Uni- 
versity, Dallas, Tex. 

MOHLER, HENRY C., 983 i8th Ave., S.E., 
Minneapolis, Minn. 

MONACHESI, ELIO D., K. of C. Hall, 
Columbia, Mo. 

MONSEES, CARL H., JR., 7301 German- 
town Ave., Mount Airy, Phila- 
delphia, Pa. 

MONTAGUE, J. F., Robinson Hotel, Co- 
lumbia, Mo. 

MONTGOMERY, EDWARD W., Knoxville 
College, KnoxviUe, Tenn. 

MONTGOMERY, LOUISE, 5611 Drexel Ave., 
Chicago, 111. 

MOODY, RUHAMA EVELYN, 15 Lena St., 
Atlanta, Ga. 

MOORE, MRS. ARTHUR A., 3520 E. Clay 
St., Richmond, Va. 

MOORE, CLYDE B., University of Pitts- 
burgh, Pittsburgh, Pa. 

MOORE, COYLE E., Box 734, Chapel Hill, 

N.C. 
MOORE, ELON H., 740 Langdon St., 

Madison, Wis. 

MOORE, HARRY H., U.S. Public Health 

Service, Washington, D.C. 
MOORE, MRS. M. D., 5858 Magnolia Ave., 

Chicago, III. 

MOORE, MARGARET, Thorntown, Ind. 
MORGAN, E. L., University of Missouri, 

Columbia, Mo. 
MORRELL, H. PHILBROOK, University 

Ave., Canton, N.Y. 
MORRIS, ANDREW FRANCIS, La Grange, 

Mo. 
MORRISON, W ALTER k., 633 Ann St., 

Kansas City, Kan. 



MEMBERSHIP LIST 



255 



MORROW, CURTIS H., 3 West Court. 

Waterville, Me. 
MORROW, VERLE, 1142 S. Curson Ave., 

Los Angeles, Calif. 
MORSE, H. N., 156 sth Ave., New York, 

N.Y. 
MOUNTS, LEWIS H., Ballard Normal 

School, Macon, Ga. 
MOWRER, ERNEST R., 5857 Blackstone 

Ave., Chicago, 111. 
MUDGE, G. O., Superintendent of Public 

Schoolsf Trenton, N.J. 
MUELLER, JOHN H., University of Mis- 
souri, Columbia, Mo. 
MUMFORD, EBEN, Michigan Agricultural 

College, East Lansing, Mich. 
MURCHIE, ROBERT W., 355 Rosedale 

Ave., Winnipeg, Can. 
MURDOCK, J. M., Polk State School, Polk, 

Pa. 
MURPHY, JOSEPH P., 52 Niagara St., 

Buffalo, N.Y. 

MURRAY, MARGARET, Mount Carroll, 111. 
MUZUMDAR, HARIDAS T., 1912 Sherman 

Ave., Evans ton, 111. 
MYERS, EARL D., 27 Woodbridge St., 

South Hadley, Mass. 
MYERS, REV. GEORGE BOGGAN, Univer 

sity of the South, Sewanee, Tenn. 
MYHRMAN, ANDERS M., University of 

Chicago, Chicago, 111. 
MYRICK, HELEN L., 162 E. Ontario St., 

Chicago, 111. 
NEALEY, E. M., R.F.D. No. i, Santa Ana, 

Calif. 
NEILL, CHARLES P., 616 Woodward Bldg., 

Washington, D.C. 
NELLES, FRED C., Whittier State School, 

Whittier, Calif. 

NELSON, LOWRY, Brigham Young Uni- 
versity, Provo, Utah 
NERDRUM, RUTH, 1127 Bowen Court, 

Madison, Wis. 
NEUMANN, GEORGE BRADFORD, Wallace 

Lodge, Yonkers, N.Y. 
NEUMEYER, M. H., 4949 Indiana Ave., 

Chicago, 111. 

NEVILS, W. COLEMAN, Georgetown Uni- 
versity, Washington, D.C. 
NEWELL, JANE I., Wellesley College, 

Wellesley, Mass. 

NEWSOM, VIDA, 820 Franklin St., Colum- 
bus, Ind. 



NEWSTETTER, WILBUR IRVIN, 2571 E. 

46th St., Cleveland, Ohio 
NICHOLS, C. A., 3425 University Blvd., 

Dallas, Tex. 

NICKOLAUS, H. F., P.O. Box 507, Allen- 
town, Pa. 
NIEMAN, L. W., c/o Milwaukee Journal, 

Milwaukee, Wis. 
NIEMI, CLEMENS, 183 N. Oliver Ave., 

Minneapolis, Minn. 
NISHINOIRI, JOHN ISAO, 4115 i$th Ave., 

N.E., Seattle, Wash. 
NIXON, JUSTIN W., Brighton Heights, 

Brighton Station, Rochester, N.Y. 
NOFCIER, WILLIAM LINCOLN, 524 S. Van 

Buren St., Iowa City, Iowa 
NORGORD, GLADYS M., 396 Quail St., 

Albany, N.Y. 
NORMAN, CHARLES G., 2204 Queen Ave., 

Seattle, Wash. 

NORRIS, ORLAND O., Ypsilanti, Mich. 
NORTH, CECIL C., Page Hall, Ohio State 

University, Columbus, Ohio 
NORTHCOTT, CLARENCE H., Cocoa Works, 

York, England 

NYSTROM, PAUL H., 333 Main St., Ridge- 
field, N.J. 

O'CoNNELL, REV. D., St. Xavier College, 
Evan Station, Cincinnati, Ohio 

O'CONNOR, ANNETTE, 15 E. Gilraan St., 
Madison, Wis. 

ODUM, HOWARD W., University of North 
Carolina, Chapel Hill, N.C. 

OGBURN, WILLIAM F., Barnard Col- 
lege, Columbia University, New 
York, N.Y. 

O'GRADY, JOHN O., Catholic University 
of America, Washington, D.C. 

O'HARA, EDWIN, 272 W. nth Ave., 
Eugene, Ore. 

OLANDER, ELMER A., 720 W. Cherokee 
St., Okmulgee, Okla. 

OLDINGS, WILLIAM, 1020 Regent St., 
Madison, Wis. 

OLIVER, JOHN R., Latrobe Apartments, 
Baltimore, Md. 

OLSEN, Esther 9106 Hamilton Ave., De- 
troit, Mich. 

OPPENHEIMER, J. J., Stephens Junior Col- 
lege, Columbia, Mo. 

ORMOND, JESSE MARVIN, 301 Watts St., 
Durham, N.C. 



256 



THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 



OSCHSNER, A. J., 2106 Sedgwick Ave., 

Chicago, 111. 
OSHIMO, RAYMOND K., 5757 University 

Ave., Chicago, 111. 
PAGE, JAMES FRANKLIN, 1315 Chandler 

St., Madison, Wis. 
PAGE, KIRBY, 311 Division Ave., Has- 

brouck Heights, N J. 
PALMER, GLADYS M., Barnard Hall, 

University of Wisconsin, Madison, 

Wis. 
PALMER, Lois E.,, Chadbourne Hall, 

Madison, Wis. 
PANUNZIO, C. M., 35 Beekman Place, 

New York, N.Y. 
PARISH, B. ERROL, 400 Pennsylvania St., 

Lincoln, Neb. 

PARK, JAMES W,, Adelphi College, Brook- 
lyn, N.Y. 

PARK, ROBERT E., University of Chicago, 
Chicago, 111. 

PARKER, WILLIAM H., 23-25 E. gth St., 
Cincinnati, Ohio 

PARRISH, C. H., 847-6 th St., Louisville, 

Ky. 
PARSONS, PHILIP A., 625 Court House, 

Portland, Ore. 

PASSAMANECK, HERMAN, 1600 Lin wood 
Blvd., Kansas City, Mo. 

PATRICK, JAMES G., Whitworth College, 
Spokane, Wash. 

PATTEN, FRANK C., Rosenberg Library, 
Galveston, Tex. 

PATTERSON, GAYLARD H., 57 S. College 
St., Carlisle, Pa. 

PATTERSON, HERBERT P., School of Edu- 
cation, Oklahoma Agricultural and 
Mechanic College, Stillwater, Okla. 

PATTISON, FRANCIS WAYLAND, Box 206, 
East Northfield, Mass. 

PAUSTIAN, ERWIN CARL, 516 S. Minne- 
sota St., Mitchell, S.D. 

PEARSON, JOHN A., Merchants National 
Bank Bldg., St. Paul, Minn. 

PENN, W. F., Superintendent, Pennsyl- 
vania Training School, Morganza, 
Pa. 

PENNOYER, CHARLES H., 176 Newbury 
St., Boston, Mass. 



PERSON, P. H,, 1627 Monroe St., Madi- 
son, Wis. 

PESKIND, DR. A., 12629 Euclid Ave., East 
Cleveland, Ohio 

PETERS, CHARLES C., 96 Montrose Ave., 
Delaware, Ohio 

PETERS, IVA L., Goucher College, Balti- 
more, Md. 

PETERSON, C. PETER A., 158 Montague 
St., Brooklyn, N.Y. 

PETTERSON, G. S., 223 W. Lewis St., 
Mankato, Minn. 

PETTYJOHN, ALETHA, 2366 Park Ave., 
Indianapolis, Ind. 

PFLEIDERER, F. A., 2330 N. Halsted St., 
Chicago, 111. 

PHELPS, HAROLD A., 10 Folwell Hall, 
University of Minnesota, Minne- 
apolis, Minn. 

PHILIP, WILLIAM B., 5315 Drexel Ave., 
Chicago, 111. 

PHILLIPS, DR. HERBERT E., 1609 W. 
Garfield Blvd., Chicago, III. 

PIERCE, GERTRUDE H., 827 Ridge Ave., 
Evanston, 111. 

PIHLBLAD, TERENCE, Lindsborg, Kan. 

PIPER, FRANCIS EDWARD, 510 S. Governor 
St., Iowa City, Iowa 

PITTMAN, MARVIN S., 502 W. Forest Ave., 
Ypsilanti, Mich. 

POUND, ROSCOE, Harvard Law School, 
Cambridge, Mass. 

POWELL, HANNAH B. CLARK, 5227 Black- 
stone Ave., Chicago, 111. 

POWELL, RICHARD WILLARD, 607 W. 2$th 
St., Kearney, Neb. 

POWELL, RUTH M., Social Service League, 
Iowa City, Iowa 

POWERS, HELENE, 454 Huntington Ave., 
Suite 1 8, Boston, Mass. 

PRATT, ANNA B., 3804 Locust Ave., Phila- 
delphia, Pa. 

PRATT, NATHANIEL M., 128 Ontario St., 
Lockport, N.Y. 

PRENTISS, MARION C., 3543 Van Buren 
St., Chicago, 111. 

PRENTISS, Z. Ransom, Oberlin, Ohio 

PRESTON, REV. J. J., Tecumseh, Mich. 

PRICE, M. T.j c/o Edward Evans & Sons, 
Shanghai, China 



PERCEFULL, SABLN C., 803 sth Ave., PRIDE, NETTIE J., 960 Parkview, Los 



Alva, Okla. 
PEKRY, CLARENCE A., 130 E. 22d St., 
New York, N.Y. 



Angeles, Calif. 
PUCKETT, NEWBELL N., Western Re- 
serve University, Cleveland, Ohio 



MEMBERSHIP LIST 



257 



PUNKE, EDWARD G., 1023 Franklin St., 
Cedar Falls, Iowa 

PUTNAM, BERTHA HAVEN, Mount Holy- 
oke College, South Hadley, Mass. 

QUEEN, STUART A., University of Kansas, 
Lawrence, Kan. 

QUIJANO, J. RIVERO, P.O. Box 1542, 
Mexico City, Mexico 

QUINN, JAMES A., University of Cincin- 
nati, Cincinnati, Ohio 

RADDANT, Louis A., 528 W. Johnson St., 
MadisSn, Wis. 

RAILEY, J. L., Mercer University, Macon, 
Ga. 

RAINWATER, CLARENCE, University of 
Southern California, Los Angeles, 
Calif. 

RAMSPERGER, H. G., 400 Allaire Ave., 
Leonia, N.J. 

RANCK, SAMUEL H., Public Library, 
Grand Rapids, Mich. 

RANDOLPH, E. D., School of Education, 
University of Washington, Seattle, 
Wash. 

RANDOLPH, E. F., 1654 Massachusetts 
Ave., Cambridge, Mass. 

RANKIN, J. O., Department of Rural Eco- 
nomics, College of Agriculture, Lin- 
coln, Neb. 

RAPKING, A. H., Buckhannon, W.Va. 

RATCLIFFE, SAMUEL C., 201 N. McCul- 
lough Ave., Urbana, 111. 

RATH, JAMES A., P.O. Box 514, Honolulu, 

Hawaii 
RAVITCH, DAVID, 3141 Dupont Ave., S., 

Minneapolis, Minn. 
RAVITCH, JESSIE, 3141 Dupont Ave., S., 

Minneapolis, Minn. 
RAY, MRS. GEORGE W., 6040 Harper 

Ave., Chicago, 111. 
RECKLESS, W. C., Vanderbilt University, 

Nashville, Tenn. 
REDMOND, L. L., Bethel College, Newton, 

Kan. 

REED, ELLERY F., 326 W. Vine St., Ox- 
ford, Ohio 

REEVES, GEORGE W., Atlanta University, 
Atlanta, Ga. 

REEVES, JAMES A., Seton Hill College, 
Greensburg, Pa. 

REINEMUND, J. A., Box 95, Muscatine, 
Iowa 

REINER, JOSEPH, 6525 Sheridan Road, 
Chicago, 111. 



REITMAN, DR. BEN L., 741 Cornelia Ave., 
Chicago, 111. 

RENNIE, WESLEY F., 4332 University 
Way, Seattle, Wash. 

REUTER, E. B., College of Commerce, 
University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa 

REYNOLDS, CHARLES N., University of 
Oregon, Portland, Ore. 

RICE, Rev. JOHN H. J., First Congrega- 
tional Church, Emporia, Kan. 

RICE, STUART A., 19 N. Main St., Han- 
over, N.H. 

RICH, CHESTER LACOUNT, Dakota Wes- 
leyan University, Mitchell, S.D. 

RICH, MRS. G. J., 5458 Ellis Ave., Chi- 
cago, 111. 

RlCKENBACHER, MABEL C., 1425 F St., 

Lincoln, Neb. 
RICKERT, HARVEY LEHMAN, 514 Keokuk 

St., Lincoln, 111. 
RIETZ, ELMER WEBBER, 5246 Glen wood 

Ave., Chicago, 111. 
RILEY, AILEEN, Salem, N.J. 
RILEY, THOMAS J., 69 Schermerhorn St., 

Brooklyn, N.Y. 
Riss, ARTHUR H., 165 Valley St., Seattle, 

Wash. 

RITCHIE, C. S., 727 S. Division St., Ann 
Arbor, Mich. 

RITTER, WILLIAM E., 1016 Spruce St., 
Berkeley, Calif. 

ROBBINS, C. L., 1049 Woodlawn Ave., 
Iowa City, Iowa 

ROBERTS, HAZEL VAN DYKE, 420 High 
St., Morgantown, W.Va. 

ROBINSON, CLARENCE C., 347 Madison 
Ave., New York, N.Y. 

ROBINSON, MILLARD L., 316 W. 79th St., 
New York, N.Y. 

ROBINSON, VIRGINIA, New Social Serv- 
ice Bldg., 311 Juniper St., Phila- 
delphia, Pa. 

ROCKEY, CARROLL J., 33 N. Chapel St., 

Elgin, Hi. 
ROEANDT, AUGUST, 59-63 2d St., New 

York, N.Y. 
ROEDER, HERBERT J., 359 $th Ave., 

New York, N.Y. 
ROESCH, FLORENCE N., 619 Langdon St., 

Madison, Wis. 

ROMINGER, CHARLES H., Bethlehem, Pa. 
ROOT, ROBERT C., College of the Pacific, 

Stockton, Calif. 



258 



THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 



ROPER, M. WESLEY, 1402 E. 6ist Place, 
Chicago, 111. 

ROSEN, BEN, 330 S. gth St., Philadelphia, 
Pa. 

ROSEN WALD, JULIUS, c/o Seats Roebuck 
& Company, Chicago, 111. 

Ross, EDWARD A., University of Wis- 
consin, Madison, Wis. 

Ross, FRANK A., Kent Hall, Columbia 
University, New York, N.Y. 

Ross, J. ELLIOT, St. Austin's Chapel, 
2010 Guadalupe St., Austin, Tex. 

Ross, SARAH GRIDLEY, Skidmore College, 
Saratoga Springs, N.Y. 

ROSSENOW, GEORGE S. H., c/o D. G. 
Conradie, Box 10, Reitz, O.F.S., 
Union of South Africa 

ROWLEY, MRS. CLARMAE BUDDE, Box 12, 
Ravenswood, W.Va. 

RUECKERT, FREDERICK, 34 Alumni Ave., 
Providence, R.I. 

RUML, BEARDSLEY, Greenfield Ave., 
Bronxville, N.Y. 

RUNNEMAN, A., 103 Gates Hall, Univer- 
sity of Chicago, Chicago, 111. 

RUSSELL, DANIEL, Baylor University, 
Waco, Texas 

RUYLE, WILLIAM LEROY, 1202 E. i8th 
St., University Place, Neb. 

SACKETT, HERMAN, Edinboro, Pa. 

SAMMIS, MRS. FLORA E., 234 Breeze 
Terrace, Madison, Wis. 

SANDAGER, FREDERICK S., Ripton, Ala. 

SANDERS, C. F., 135 Broadway, Gettys- 
burg, Pa. 

SANDERS, J. T., Oklahoma Agricultural 
and Mechanical College, Stillwater, 
Okla. 

SANDERS, WILEY B., 1404 E. syth St., 
Chicago, 111. 

SANDERSON, D WIGHT, 212 Overlook Road, 
Ithaca, N.Y. 

SANTAYANO, SILVIO, 688 Boylston St., 
Boston, Mass. 

SAPOSNEKOW, JACOB, West Virginia Uni- 
versity, Morgantown, W.Va. 

SAUER, M. LUELLA, 1449 S. Fort Thomas 
Ave., Fort Thomas, Ky. 

SAUL, HERBERT W., 59 Pleasant St., Lex- 
ington, Mass. 

SCHAFF, MATILDA C., 601 N. Pine Ave., 
Chicago, 111. 

SCHAUB, E. L., 629 Garrett Place, Evans- 
ton, 111. 



SCHIVELY, MARY ALICE, 318 Winona 
Ave., Philadelphia, Pa. 

SCHLAFENHAUF, MlLTON J., 316 Hunting- 

ton Ave., Boston, 17, Mass. 
SCHMID, CALVIN F., 3410 Burke Ave., 

Seattle, Wash. 

SCHROEDER, 303 Callender St., Peoria, 111. 
SCHROER, LUCRETIA, ioo9 Laurel Ave., 

St. Paul, Minn. 

SCHULTZ, CHARLES H., M.A., Box 36, 
Loretto, Pa. 

SCOTT, ELMER, 416 Mercantile Bank 
Bldg., Dallas, Tex. 

SCOTT, H. MILLER, Superior, Neb. 
SCRITSMIER, Menomonie, Wis. 

SEABURG, ANNA, National Board, 
Y.W.C.A.'s, 600 Lexington Ave., 
New York, N.Y. 

SEARS, A. G., 7509-88^ Ave., Wood- 
haven, L.I., N.Y. 

SEMAN, PHILIP L., Jewish Peoples Insti- 
tute, 1258 W. Taylor St., Chicago, 
111. 

SEESE, NORMAN A., 5800 Maryland Ave., 
Chicago, 111. 

SELIGMAN, E. R. A., 324 W. 86th St., 
New York, N.Y. 

SELINGER, HUGO P. J., North Platte , Neb. 

SELISKAR, John, St. Paul Seminary, Grove- 
land Park, St. Paul, Minn. 

SELL, HARRY B., Reed College, Portland, 
Ore. 

SELLE, ERWIN STEVENSON, State Teach- 
ers College, Winona, Minn. 

SELLIN, J. THORSTEN, Wharton School, 
University of Pennsylvania, Phila- 
delphia, Pa. 

SELVAGE, WATSON, 924^ 6th Ave., 
Huntington. W. Va. 

SENEY, HENRY W., II, 3008 Collingwood 
Ave., Toledo, Ohio 

SETTERLUND, ELMER LEONARD, Reddick, 
111. 

SEYMOUR, A. H., Northern Normal In- 
dustrial School, Aberdeen, S.D. 

SHANE, WILLIAM L. E., University Sta- 
tion, Enid, Okla. 

SHAPER, FLORENCE W., Lindenwood Col- 
lege, St. Charles, Mo. 

SHARPLESS, HELEN, Haverford College, 
Haverford, Pa. 

SHAW, FLORENCE, Shepherdstown, W.Va. 



MEMBERSHIP LIST 



SHEA, REV. GEORGE H., Quarryville, Pa. 
SHEAHAN, MARIE, Loyola University, 

Rogers Park, Chicago, 111. 
SHEETZ, LE ROY ARTHUR, Hastings Col- 
lege, Hastings, Neb. 
SHEFFIELD, MRS. ADA E., 60 Shepard St., 

Cambridge, Mass. 
SHELDON, ANNA M., 1207 W. $d St., Los 

Angeles, Calif. 
SHELLENBERGER, HELEN O., 4511 Pine 

St., Philadelphia, Pa. 
SHELLENBERGER, JAMES KNOX, 1025 N. 

Saunders Ave., Bethany, Neb. 
SHELTMAN, EMMY Lou, 2227 Alta Ave., 

Louisville, Ky. 
SHENTON, CLARENCE G., 311 S. Juniper 

St., Philadelphia, Pa. 
SHENTON, H. N., Columbia University, 

New York, N.Y. 
SHEPARD, ROBERT L., Missouri Valley 

College, Marshall, Mo. 
SHERIDAN, JONES T., 625 N. Henry St., 

Madison, Wis. 
SHIDELER, ERNEST H., 98 S. Home Ave., 

Franklin, Ind. 
SHONLE, RUTH, 5643 Dorchester Ave., 

Chicago, 111. 

SHRODER, WILLIAM J., 25 E. gth St., Cin- 
cinnati, Ohio 
SHROYER, MONTGOMERY J., College Ave., 

Westminster, Md. 
SIBLEY, FLORENCE, 1937 Panama St., 

Philadelphia, Pa. 
SIDLO, THOMAS L., Union National Bank 

Bldg., Cleveland, Ohio 
SIEDENBURG, FREDERIC, 617 Ashland 

Block, Clark and Randolph St., Chi- 
cago, 111. 
SIMMONS, CATHERINE, 2805 Wool worth 

Ave., Omaha, Neb. 
SIMMONS, Miss H. M., 688 Boylston St., 

Boston, Mass. 
SIMMS, STOCKWELL, 177 Webster St., 

East Boston, Mass. 
SIMPSON, EYLER NEWTON, University of 

Chicago, Chicago, 111. 
SISTER M. IRENAEUS, Rosalia Foundling 

Asylum and Maternity Hospital, 

Pittsburgh, Pa. 
SISTER ROSE DE LIMA, College of St. 

Elizabeth, Convent, N.J. 
SIMS, NEWELL L., Oberlin, Ohio 
SINGER, JACOB. 6932 N. Ashland Ave., 

Chicago, 111. 



SKINNER, CLARENCE R., Packard Hall, 
Tufts College, Mass. 

SLY, ALTHO G., Live Oak, Calif. 

SMALL, ALBION W., University of Chi- 
cago, Chicago, 111. 

SMITH, DOROTHY ALICE, 4135 Brooklyn 
Ave., Seattle, Wash. 

SMITH, DOROTHY WELLINGTON, 626 
Center St., Newton, Mass. 

SMITH, HELEN FRANCES, 264 Bay State 
Road, Boston, Mass. 

SMITH, E. S., Callender, Iowa 

SMITH, FREDERICK M., P.O. Box 255, 
Independence, Mo. 

SMITH, IGNATIUS, Dominican Home of 
Studies, 487 Michigan Ave., N.E., 
Washington, D.C. 

SMITH, L. C., 35 Reed Hall, Dartmouth 
College, Hanover, N.H. 

SMITH, MAURICE, Box 1125, Station A, 
Lincoln, Neb. 

SMITH, RUSSELL GORDON, Furnald Hall, 
Columbia University, New York, 
N.Y. 

SMITH, TUCKER P., Y.M.C.A., Spring- 
field College, Springfield, Mass. 

SMITH, WALTER R., University of Kansas, 
Lawrence, Kan. 

SMITH, WILLIAM C., University of South- 
ern California, Los Angeles, Calif, 

SMITH, W. WILBERFORCE, 151 N. Fair- 
view Ave., Decatur, 111. 

SMITHEY, W. R., University, Va. 

SNAVELY, WILLIAM REX, 805 N. Market 
Ave., Canton, Ohio 

SNEDDEN, DAVID, Teachers College, 
Columbia University, New York, 
N.Y. 

SNELL, CHESTER D., University Exten- 
sion Division, Chapel Hill, N.C. 

SNYDER, H. M., 7514 Constance Ave., 
Chicago, 111. 

SOBEY, JAMES H., 720 3d Ave., Eau 
Claire, Wis. 

SODERSTEN, BESSIE C., $21 N. Van Buren 
St., Iowa City, Iowa 

SOLENBERGER, EDWIN D., 7th Floor, 

Social Service Bldg., 311 S. Juniper 
St., Philadelphia, Pa. 

SOROKIN, PITIRIM, University of Minne- 
sota, Minneapolis, Minn. 

SPAETH, J. DUNCAN, Princeton, N.J. 

SPALDING, HENRY S., 626 N. Vandeventer 
Ave., St. Louis, Mo. 



THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 



SPENCER, MRS. ANNA GARLIN, 255 W. 
97th St., New York, N.Y. 

SPENCER, CHARLOTTE, 7 Temple St., 
Cambridge, Mass. 

SPENCER, FRANK G., Lewis Institute, 
Chicago, III. 

SPLITTER, HENRY W., 2230 Hollister St., 
Madison, Wis. 

SPYKMAN, V., 107 Whitney Ave., Berke- 
ley, Calif. 

STAACK, HENRY F., 309 E. Church St., 
Iowa City, Iowa 

STAGEY, W. H., Iowa State College of Ag- 
riculture and Mechanical Arts, Ames, 
Iowa 

STACK, WICKLIFFE, 1817 W. 43d Place, 
Los Angeles, Calif. 

STACY, W. H., Iowa State College, Ames, 
Iowa 

STAHL, SARAH STARR, 1655 E. 67th St., 
Chicago, 111. 

STANTON, WILLIAM THURMAN, Baylor 
College, Belton, Tex. 

STEINER, B. C., Enoch Pratt Library, 
Baltimore, Md. 

STEINER, J. F., University of North 
Carolina, Chapel Hill, N.C. 

STEPANEK, DR. B., Hotel Pennsylvania, 
New York, N.Y. 

STEPHAN, FREDERICK FRANKLIN, 516 S. 
Carpenter Ave., Oak Park, 111. 

STEPHENSON, MRS. F. W., 411 Standard 
Bldg., Fort Wayne, Ind. 

STETSER, MARGARET C., Chester High 
School, Chester, Pa. 

STEWARD, FLORENCE MARIE, Apt. 3, 
The Norwood, i9th St., Toledo, 
Ohio 

STEWART, ANNA, 852 W. 3$th Place, Los 
Angeles, Calif. 

STEWART, REV. CARLYLE F., 3119 i8th 
Ave., S., Minneapolis, Minn. 

STEWART, CHARLES GIDEON, 107 E. Main 
St., Norwalk, Ohio 

STEWART, ETHELBERT, 1210 Delafield 
Place, N.W., Washington, D.C. 

STEWART, MRS. MABEL EATON, Monti- 
cello Seminary, Godfrey, 111. 

STOOD ARD, LOTHROP, 1768 Beacon St., 
Brookline, Mass. 

STONE, W. B., Commerce, Tex. 

STOVER, HELEN H., 934 W. 34th St., Los 
Angeles, Calif. 



STRATER, CHARLES G., 350 Madison Ave., 
New York, N.Y. 

STREET, ELWOOD, 511 Locust St., St. 
Louis, Mo. 

STRIDSBERG, Rev., CARL, 424 Burns Ave., 
Cincinnati, Ohio 

STRONG, HERSEY H., Atlanta University, 
Atlanta, Ga. 

STROTHER, W., Louisiana College, Pine- 
ville, La. 

STROW, CARL W., Knox College, Gales- 
burg, 111. 

STUBBS, FLORENCE H., 703 High St., 
Farmville, Va. 

STURGES, HERBERT ARTHUR, 5707 Ken- 
wood Ave., Chicago, 111. 

SULLINGER, J. EARL, 2723 Pinkney St., 
Omaha, Neb. 

SULLIVAN, C. M., State Normal School, 
Lock Haven, Pa. 

SULLIVAN, DANIEL R., Seton Hill College, 
Greensburg, Pa. 

SUMMERHILL, JOSEPH ARTHUR, Leonard, 
Tex. 

SUMNER, G. S., Pomona College, Clare- 
mont, Calif. 

SUMNER, MARY CLAYTON, Berkshire In- 
dustrial Farm, Canaan, N.Y. 

SUN, P. W. B., International House, 
Riverside Drive, New York, N.Y. 

SUSSMAN, JEROME, 50 W. 77th St., New 

York, N.Y. 
SUTHERLAND, E. H., Room 314, Lincoln 

Hall, University of Illinois, Urbana, 

SUTTON, BESSIE E., 606 N. Francis St., 
Madison, Wis. 

SUZZALLO, Henry, University of Wash- 
ington, Seattle, Wash. 

SWIFT, HAROLD H., Union Stock Yards, 
Chicago, 111. 

SWIFT, ROY FREDERICK, Rockford Col- 
lege, Rockford, 111. 

SYDNEY, ELIAS CHARLES, 951 Avenue St. 
John, New York, N.Y. 

SYMOND, ARTHUR F., Yamagachi Higher 
Commercial School, Yamagachi, 
Japan 

TAFT, DONALD R., Wells College, Aurora, 
N.Y. 

TALBOT, HOMER, 526 Clarke Ave., Jeffer- 
son City, Mo, 

TALBOT, MARION, University of Chicago, 
Chicago, 111. 



MEMBERSHIP LIST 



261 



TATT, S. H., 99 Gates Hall, University of 
Chicago, Chicago, 111. 

TAUSIG, FRANCES, 125 E. 46th St., New 
York, N. Y. 

TAYLOR, CARL C, State College of Agri- 
culture and Engineering, West Ra- 
leigh, N.C. 

TAYLOR, F. B., Jamestown College, 
Jamestown, N.D. 

TAYLOR, GRAHAM, Chicago Commons, 
Grand Ave. and Morgan St., Chi- 
cago, Bi. 

TAYLOR, MAURICE, 6 N. Russell St., 
Boston, Mass. 

TAWNEY, G. A., University of Cincinnati, 
Cincinnati, Ohio 

TEETER, VIRL H., 204 E. Fairchild St., 
Iowa City, Iowa 

TEMPLIN, LUCINDA DE L., 705 Missouri 
Ave., Columbia, Mo. 

TENNEY, ALVAN A., Columbia University, 
New York, N.Y. 

TERRELL, NELLIE EMMA, 75 Chester St., 
Allston, Mass. 

TETREAU, E., The Wesley Foundation, 
Madison, Wis. 

THADEN, JOHN F., Iowa State College, 
Ames, Iowa 

THOMAS, HARRIET F., Atlanta University, 
Atlanta, Ga. 

THOMAS, MARJORIE M., Fairfield, Iowa 

THOMAS, PAUL B., c/o Alfred A. Knopf, 
730 5th Ave., New York, N.Y. 

THOMPSON, MRS. AGNES L., Box 4, 
Gurnee, 111. 

THOMPSON, MILTON WINFIELD, Coving- 
ington, Ind. 

THOMPSON, WARREN S., Miami Univer- 
sity, Oxford, Ohio 

THORNDIKE, F. L., Teachers College, 
^ Columbia University, New York, N.Y. 

THRASHER, FREDERIC M., Illinois Wes- 
leyan University, Bloomington, 111. 

THRELKELD, ARCHIE LOYD, 1153 S. Gay- 
lord St., Denver, Colo. 

THURMAN, Miss E., 688 Boylston St., 
Boston, Mass. 

TIBBITTS, ROY C., 4143 N. Tripp Ave., 
Chicago, 111. 

TINNEY, MARY C., 258 Wilioughby Ave., 
Brooklyn, N.Y. 

TODA, TEIZO, Department of Sociology, 
Imperial University of Tokyo, To- 
kyo, Japan 



TODD, A. J., 415 $. Franklin St., Chicago, 
111, 

Toi, KWEN L, 128 South Divinity Hall, 
University of Chicago, Chicago, 111. 

TOLLENS, A. T., 17 Harvard St., Ver- 
million, N.D. 

TOPPING, C. W., College of Puget Sound, 
Tacoma, Wash. 

TORRANCE, MARY, Muncie Public Li- 
brary, Muncie, Ind. 

TOURTELOT, IDA A., Mohonk School, 
Mohonk Lake, N.Y. 

TOWER, C. V., Collegeville, Pa. 

TOWNE, ARTHUR W., 1213 James St., 
Syracuse, N.Y. 

TOWNE, E. T., University Station, Grand 
Forks, N.D. 

TOWNE, MRS. MARIE REED, 444 S. Hope 
St., Los Angeles, Calif. 

TOWNLEY, LUELLA, The Chesterfield, 
900 W. Franklin St., Richmond, Va. 

TOWNSEND, H. R., High School, Hamil- 
ton, Ohio 

TOWNSEND, MRS. MARGARET, Hotel 
Montgomery, Clarksville, Tenn. 

TRENT, Miss ALTON A M., 52 Johnson 
Ave., Atlanta, Ga. 

TROUT, G. W., Kansas State Teachers 
College, Pittsburg, Kan. 

TUNG, J. C. SHIH, Nan Kai University, 
Tientsin, China 

TUNNICLIFPE, R. M., State Normal Col 
lege, Bowling Green, Ohio 

TURNER, W. S., Office of the Dean, Shaw 
University, Raleigh, N.C. 

TURNER-HIGH, HARRY, The University 
Club, Madison, Wis. 

TUTHILL, GRETCHEN L., 7320 Hollywood 
Blvd., Los Angeles, Calif. 

TYLER, W. RUSSELL, Knox College, Gales- 
burg, 111. 

TYSON, FRANCIS, University at Pitts- 
burgh, Pittsburgh, Pa. 

UCHIYAMA, GENICHI, Higashagata, Ku- 
wana Island, Japan 

UESUGI, SUMIO, 119 W. 87th St., New 
York, N.Y. 

UNDERWOOD, MRS. FRANK L., 124 Morn- 
ingside, Ferger Place, Chattanooga, 
Tenn. 

UNDERWOOD, J. H., University of Mon- 
tana, Missoula, Mont. 

UNGER, MRS. J. 0., 634 Grace St., Chi- 
cago, 111. 



262 



THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 



VAN BUSKIRK, EDGAR F., 25 E. gth St., 
Cincinnati, Ohio 

VAN DRIEL, AGNES, 742 Addison St., 
Chicago, III. 

VAN DUSEN, A. P., University of Syra- 
cuse, Syracuse, N.Y. 

VAN TRUMP, CLARA JESSUP, Kalarama 
Road, Washington, B.C. 

VASSARDAKIS, CLEANTHES, 88 Hope St., 
Ridgewood, NJ. 

VEH, RAYMOND M., 514 E. Daniel St., 
Champaign, 111. 

VEILLER, LAWRENCE, 102 E. 22d St., 
New York, N.Y. 

VETESK, A. D., 1611 Chadbourne Ave., 
Madison, Wis. 

VILLA-REAL, ANT., Attorney General, 
Bureau of Justice, Manila, P.I. 

VINCENT, GEORGE E., Rockefeller Foun- 
dation, 6 1 Broadway, New York, 
N.Y. 

VINCENT, MELVIN J., 1616 N. Mariposa 
Ave., Los Angeles, Calif. 

VOGT, PAUL L., 1701 Arch St., Phila- 
delphia, Pa. 

VOLD, GEORGE B., Macalester College, 
St. Paul, Minn. 

VOLTMER, HARRY W., State Historical 
Society, Iowa City, Iowa 

VON TUNGELN, GEORGE H., Iowa State 
College, Ames, Iowa 

VOORHIS, EDWARD B., 3712 Locust St., 
Philadelphia, Pa. 

WALDKOENIG, REV. ARTHUR, 113 Gates 
Hall, University of Chicago, Chi- 
cago, 111. 

WALDKOENIG, H. A., Room 32, City 
Hall, Battle Creek, Mich. 

WALKER, EDWIN C., 211 W. i38th St., 
New York, N.Y. 

WALKINSHAW, RUTH IONE, Oliver, 
Mich. 

WALLIS, WILSON DALLAM, University of 
Minnesota, Minneapolis, Minn. 

WANG, TSE C., 5757 University Ave., 
Chicago, 111. 

WARFIELD, G. A., 2131 S. Columbine St., 
Denver, Colo. 

WARGELIN, JOHN, Suomi College, Han- 
cock, Mich. 

WARNER, MASON, 260 N. Michigan Blvd., 
Chicago, 111. 

WARREN, HOWARD C., Princeton, NJ. 



WATANUKI, TETSUO, Tokio Higher Nor- 
mal School, Tokio, Japan 

WATERS, CHESTER C., Three Pine Ave., 
Takoma Park, D.C. 

WATSON, FRANK D., 5 College Ave., 
Haverford, Pa. 

WATSON, MRS. FRANK D., 5 College Ave., 
Haverford, Pa. 

WATSON, MAUD E., no E. Forest St. 
Three Oaks, Mich. 

WATTERS, MARY, Montezuma College, 
East Las Vegas, N.M. 

WATTS, SHELLEY D., 515 E. 4th St., 
Bloomington, Ind. 

WEAVER, ADA E., c/o Mrs. S. B. Weld, 
Mattapoisett, Mass. 

WEATHERLY, U. G., University of Indi- 
ana, Bloomington, Ind. 

WEAVER, W. WALLACE, Logan Hall, 
University of Pennsylvania, Phila- 
delphia, Pa. 

WEBB, SAMUEL L., 6045 Pemberton Bidg., 
Boston, Mass. 

WEBBER, DR. GUSTAVUS, 1528 P St., 
N.W., Washington, D.C. 

WEBSTER, HUTTON, Station A, Lincoln, 
Neb. 

WECKWORTH, LAMBERT, St. John's Col- 
lege, Collegeville, Minn. 

WEEKS, ESTELLA T., 380 Hawthorne 
Ave., Yonkers, N.Y. 

WEEKS, RUFUS WELLS, 346 Broadway, 
New York, N.Y. 

WEH>LER, A. G., Berea College, Berea, 

Ky. 
WEIFFENBACH, EUGENE, Warrenton, Mo. 

WHEELOCK, WEBSTER, Public Library, 
St. Paul, Minn. 

WEITSMAN, Louis GABRIEL, 651 Jefferson 
St., Detroit, Mich. 

WENTWORTH, MILDRED P., Old Service 
Hall, University of Iowa, Iowa City, 
Iowa 

WESLEY, OSCAR, Syracuse University, 
Syracuse, N.Y. 

WESSEL, MRS. BESSIE BLOOM, 99 Oneco 
Ave., New London, Conn. 

WESTERFIELD, R. B., Yale University, 
New Haven, Conn. 

WEYAND, L. D., 304 E. Mississippi St., 
Liberty, Mo. 

WHEELER, JOSEPH L., Youngstown, Pub- 
lic Library, Youngstown, Ohio 



MEMBERSHIP LIST 



263 



WHELCHEL, JAMES O., 802 Black St., 
Silver City, N.M. 

WHITCOMB, SELDEN L., University of 
Kansas, Lawrence, Kan. 

WHITE, E. E., 682 Summit Ave., Mil- 
waukee, Wis. 

WHITE, LYNN TOWNSEND, Box 292, San 
Anselmo, Calif. 

WHITE, R. CLYDE, College Station, Tex. 

WILBUR, WALTER B., Charleston, S.C. 

WILDER, CONSTANCE P., 53 Fairmont 
Ave., Newton, Mass. 

WILDES, H. E.. 264 Queen Lane, Ger- 
mantown, Philadelphia, Pa. 

WILEDEN, ARTHUR F., R.F.D. No. 3, 
Pewaukee, Wis. 

WILKINS, R., 688 Boylston St., Boston, 
Mass. 

WILLCOX, WALTER F., Cornell Univer- 
sity, Ithaca, N.Y. 

WILLEY, MALCOLM M., 19 N. Main St., 
Hanover, N.H. 

WILLEY, NANCY B. Boyd, Dartmouth 
College, Hanover, N.H. 

WILLIAMS, ALLEN B., S.P. Brooks Hall, 
Baylor University, Waco, Tex. 

WILLIAMS, AUBREY, Wisconsin Confer- 
ence of Social Work, University Ex- 
tension Bldg., Madison, Wis. 

WILLIAMS, G. CROFT, University of South 
Carolina, Columbia, S.C. 

WILLIAMS, H. A., 7040 Normal Ave., Chi- 
cago, 111. 

WILLIAMS, HOMER L., Box 92, Parkville, 
Mo. 

WILLIAMS, I. C., Slippery Rock, Pa. 

WILLIAMS, JOSEPH T., Whittier College, 
Whittier, Calif. 

WILLIAMS, NELSON, JR., 2100 5th Ave., 
New York, N. Y. 

WILLIAMS, MRS. T. F. A., 407 N. 26th St., 
Lincoln, Neb. 

WILLIAMSON, CHARLES C., Scenic Drive- 
way, Hastings-on-Hudson, New York 

WILLS, ALVIN LAMAR, Midway, Ky. 
WILSON, GOLD REFINED, Durmid, Va. 
WILSON, J. SCOTT, Clinton, 111. 
WILSON, J. E., 528 S. $th Ave., Chicago, 

111. 
WILSON, M. L., 2841 27th St., N.E., 

Washington, D.C. 
WILSON, WARREN H., 156 5th Ave., New 

York, N.Y. 



WINSLOW, EMMA A., 370 7th Ave., New 
York, N.Y. 

WINTERS, FRANK H., 225 Van Rensselaer 
St., Rensselaer, Ind. 

WIRTH, Louis, 5705 Kimbark Ave., Chi- 
cago, 111. 

WITHERSPOON, J. E., 98 S. Lake Ave., 
Pasadena, Calif. 

WITMER, HELEN L., 431 Hawthorne 
Court, Madison, Wis. 

WITTE, W. L., 114 S. Garth Ave., Colum- 
bia, Mo. 

WITTKAMPER, WILLIAM W., Windfall, 
Ind. 

WOLFE, A. B., Ohio State University, 
Columbus, Ohio 

WOLFERS, DR. ARNOLD, Bismarckstrasse 
21, Berlin- Wannsee, Germany 

WOOD, ALVA V., 85 E. Fayette St., 
Hillsdale, Mich. 

WOOD, L. FOSTER, 210 N. 4th St., Lewis- 
burg, Pa. 

WOODARD, JAMES W., 4329 Baltimore 
Ave., Philadelphia, Pa. 

WOODS, A. E., 43 Harvard Place, Ann 
Arbor, Mich. 

WOODRUFF, CLINTON ROGERS, North 
American Bldg., Philadelphia, Pa. 

WOODS, ERVILLE B., Hanover, N.H. 

WOOD WARD, COMER M., Southern Method- 
ist University, Dallas, Tex. 

WOODWARD, JULIAN LAURENCE, 612 W. 
n6th St., New York, N.Y. 

WOODWORTH, A. H., Hanover College, 
Hanover, Ind. 

WOOLSTON, HOWARD B., University of 
Washington, Seattle, Wash. 

WORK, MONROE N., Tuskegee Institute, 
Tuskegee, Ala. 

WRIGHT, CHARLES C., Bridgewater, Va. 

WRIGHT, CLINTON, 1020 S. Broadway, 
Pittsburg, Kan. 

WRIGHT, JONATHAN, Windy Rock, Pleas- 
antville, Westchester County, N.Y. 

Wu, CHARLES L., 508 S. 4th St., Colum- 
bia, Mo. 

Wu, WENTSAO, 45 Middle Fayerweather, 
Hanover, N.H. 

WYCKOFF, G. P., Tulane University, New 
Orleans, La. 

WYLLYS, RUFUS KAY, Kalamazoo Col- 
lege, Mich. 

YANAGIDA, KUNIO, 16-2 Kagacho Ushi- 
gomeku, Tokyo, Japan 



264 



THE AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL SOCIETY 



YING, C. Y., 710 Keng Ching Lee, Singa 
Road, Shanghai, China 

YODER, FRED R., State College of Wash- 
ington, Pullman, Wash. 

YODER, T. DALE, College of Commerce, 
University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa 

YOKOYAMA, HlDESABURO, 1500 Post St., 

San Francisco, Calif. 

YOUNG, BENJAMIN F., 62 Belvedere Place, 
Yonkers, N.Y. 

YOUNG, DONALD, Wharton School, Uni- 
versity of Pennsylvania, Phila- 
delphia, Pa. 

YOUNG, ERLE FISKE, University of South- 
ern California, Los Angeles, Calif. 

YOUNG, FREDERIC GEORGE, 599 E. 9th 
St., Eugene, Ore. 

YOUNG, KIMBALL, University of Oregon, 
Eugene, Ore. 



YOUNG, SADIE, 506 Turner Ave., Colum- 
bia, Mo. 

YTREHUS, OSCAR B., 5485 Ellis Ave., 
Chicago, 111. 

ZELENY, LESLIE D., State Teachers Col- 
lege, St. Cloud, Minn. 

ZELLER, J. C., Zelleria Plantation, Yazoo 
City, Miss. 

ZIMMERER, JOHN G., Kohler, Wis. 

ZIMMERMAN, CARLE C., Agricultural Eco- 
nomics University Farm, St. Paul, 
Minn. 

ZlMMETHAEKEL, MRS. HARRY, 630 Gilpm 

St., Denver, Colo. 

ZNANIECKI, FLORIAN, University of Poz- 
nan, Poznan, Poland 

ZORBAUGH, HARVEY W., Ohio Wesleyan 
University, Delaware, Ohio 



FEINTED IN THE U.S.A.