Skip to main content

Full text of "Wild, free-roaming horses : status of present knowledge"

See other formats


BLM   LIBRARY 


T/N    294 

Filing  Code     4700 

Date  Issued     March    19  7  7 


TECHNICAL   NOTE 


U.S.     DEPARTMENT    OF    THE    INTERIOR 
Bureau  of  Land  Management 

U.S.     DEPARTMENT    OF    AGRICULTURE 
U.S.    Forest  Service 


WILD,   FREE-ROAMING  HORSES   -  STATUS  OF  PRESENT  KNOWLEDGE 

by  Mark  Zarn,  Thomas  Heller  and 
Kay  Collins,   Research  Biologist 
Conservation  Library 
Denver  Public  Library 


jft-2 


QL 
.  L35 

no  .  294         Additional  copies  of  Technical  Notes  are  ava 


liable  from  DSC,   Federal  Center  Building  50,  Denver,   Colo..  80225 


ient 


4u         /,  «5r66&5/T  qL 


M 


TABLE  OF  CONTENTS 

Page 

Introduction  1 

Species  Description   1 

General 1 

Colors  3 

Size  of  Horses 5 

Length  of  Life 5 

Aging  Horses 5 

Locomotion  6 

Population  Characteristics  7 

Distribution  8 

Population  Status   9 

Origin  and  History  of  Wild  Horses 11 

Origin  of  Wild  Horses 11 

Domestication  of  the  Horse 12 

Influence  of  Horses  on  History  13 

The  Horse  in  America 14 

The  Wild  Horse  Controversy 15 

Reproduction  17 

Sexual  Behavior  in  Females  17 

Sexual  Behavior  in  the  Stallion .  19 

Foaling 19 

Reactions  of  Other  Horses  to  Mating   20 

Food  Habits 20 

Senses 25 

Vision 25 

Smell  and  Taste 25 

Hearing 26 

Tactile  Sensations  27 

Social  Organization  and  Behavior  28 

General 28 

Aggressiveness  29 

Family  Groups   31 

Home  Range 32 

Territoriality  34 

Vocalizations   35 

Postures  and  Facial  Expressions   35 

Grooming 37 

Play 38 


i    2  f¥ 


TABLE  OF  CONTENTS  (Cont'd) 

Page 

Sleep  and  Rest 39 

Pawing 40 

Eliminative  Behavior  40 

Scent  or  Visual  Boundary  Marking  41 

Water  and  Watering  Behavior 42 

Foal  Behavior 43 

Coprophagy  in  Foals 45 

Predation  and  Disease   45 

Predation 45 

Disease 45 

Competition  and  Relationships  with  Other  Animals  .  .  48 

Competition 48 

Relationships  with  Other  Animals  48 

Wild  Horse  Management   49 

Population  Management   49 

Population  Control  51 

Methods  of  Capture 51 

Management  Plans  53 

Current  Problems  53 

Advantages  of  Wild  Horses 53 

Disadvantages  of  Wild  Horses 54 

Research  Needs  .....  54 

Current  Research  54 

Designated  Wild  Horse  Ranges  54 

Legislation  Concerning  Wild  Horses  55 

Organizations  Concerned  With  the  Welfare  of  Wild 

Horses 55 

Glossary 56 

Appendices 57 

Literature  Cited  66 


APPENDICES 

Appendix  1  Labeled  Points  of  the  Horse 

Appendix  2   Three  Types  of  Lumbar  Vertebrae  Found  in  Wild  Horses 

Appendix  3   Coat  Color  in  Horses 

Appendix  4  Gaits  of  the  Horse,  Drawn  from  Cine  Film 

Appendix  5   Features  of  Equid  Evolution:   Skull,  Foot  Structure, 

Dentition  and  Diet 
Appendix  6  List  of  Plants  Making  up  2%  or  More  of  the  Diet  of 

Wild  Horses  in  the  Western  United  States 
Appendix  7   Historical  Distribution  of  Horses 
Appendix  8  Map  Showing  Distribution  of  Wild  Horses  in  the 

Western  United  States 


Introduction 

The  purpose  of  this  report  is  to  provide  personnel  of  the  U.  S. 
Forest  Service  and  U.  S.  Bureau  of  Land  Management  with  a 
literature  review  and  summary  of  available  information  on  wild 
horses. 

The  scientific  and  recorded  factual  information  on  America's 
wild  horses  is  extremely  limited.   Klingel  (1972),  who  has 
spent  many  years  studying  the  wild  Equidae  of  Africa,  states 
that,  except  for  social  organization,  all  members  of  the  genus 
are  very  much  alike.   As  a  result,  many  portions  of  this  tech- 
nical note  were  summarized  from  data  on  domestic  horses  or 
from  data  on  other  members  of  the  Equidae.   Therefore,  the 
validity  of  much  of  the  information  as  it  applies  to  America's 
wild  horses  will  have  to  stand  the  test  of  future  research. 

Only  a  relatively  small  number  of  people  know  very  much  about 
wild  horses.   Certain  individuals  such  as  ranchers  and  those 
few  who  have  chased  them,  lived  near,  or  been  around  them  all 
their  lives  often  possess  a  remarkable  store  of  knowledge  on 
their  habits  and  general  ecology.   Thus,  there  is  a  consider- 
able amount  of  anecdotal  information  that  cannot  be  quoted 
because  it  cannot  be  verified. 

The  problems  of  management  are  extremely  complicated.   We  are 
not  dealing  with  a  homogenous  population  of  wild  animals.   At 
the  extremes  are  those  horses  that  have  been  in  the  wild  for 
many  generations  and  are  capable  of  surviving  under  the  harshest 
of  conditions.   At  the  other  end  are  those  animals  only  a  step 
or  two  away  from  domestication  that  have  not  as  yet  fully 
adapted  to  their  environment.   Management  of  horses  as  truly 
wild  animals  would  consider  little  or  no  interference  by  man; 
however,  this  approach  could  be  hazardous  and  is  almost  an 
impossibility  under  the  increasing  pressures  for  various  land 
uses.   Other  types  of  management,  related  to  game  animals  or 
to  livestock,  are  hamstrung  by  lack  of  biological  data  and  may 
be  restricted  by  special  interest  groups  or  the  emotional  issues 
involved. 


Species  Description 

General.   The  family  Equidae,  to  which  the  horse  belongs, 
also  includes  donkeys,  zebras,  onagers  and  all  their  ancestors 
back  to  eohippus.   All  the  living  Equidae  represent  a  single 
genus,  Equus ,  with  six  species.   Domestic  horses  and  their  wild 
relatives  form  one  species;  the  asses,  both  domestic  and  wild, 
another;  the  onagers  a  third;  zebras  comprise  the  other  three 
species. 


The  genus  Equus  includes  all  members  of  the  family  Equidae 
and  their  immediate  ancestors  during  the  last  Ice  Age.   Equus 
caballus  includes  only  the  domestic  horse  and  wild  relatives  so 
closely  related  to  it  that  they  are  capable  of  interbreeding  and 
producing  fertile  offspring  (Simpson,  1951). 

The  andulusian  horse  that  came  to  America  was  reconstructed  from 
old  paintings  and  records  by  Cabrera.   The  animal  was  small  to 
medium  in  type,  rather  close  to  the  ground,  had  a  wide  chest, 
ample  barrel,  a  muscular,  rather  short  neck,  and  a  rounded 
sloping  croup  with  a  low- set  tail.   The  latter  two  character- 
istics are  also  typical  of  Barb  points  (Simpson,  1951). 

The  Spanish  Barb  Wild  Horse  Research  Farm  describes  the  Barb: 
it  is  small  (under  14  hands)  and  weighs  about  800  pounds.   The 
front  legs  join  the  deep  narrow  chest  in  a  reversed  V.   The 
withers  are  low.   The  ergots  are  small  or  non-existent,  the 
chestnuts  small,  smooth,  and  soft  and  do  not  peel  as  they  do 
on  draft  breeds.   The  croup  is  low  with  the  back  legs  well  under 
it.   The  back  is  short  with  17  pairs  of  ribs  and  either  five 
lumbar  vertebrae  or  the  sixth  fused  to  the  fifth.   The  head  is 
small,  not  over  18  inches  from  the  pole  to  the  end  of  the  nasal 
bone.   The  ears  are  small,  rimmed  with  black;  light  hair  grows 
out  of  the  center  of  the  ear.   The  muzzle  is  also  small  and 
has  crescent-shaped  nostrils. 

Colors  are  solid,  roan,  or  grulla,  which  shades  from  slate  to 
mouse  brown.   The  mane,  tail,  hooves  and  legs  below  the  knee 
are  black.   The  hair  on  the  back  of  the  legs  grows  in  a  curl 
and  comb . 

The  present-day  wild  horse,  due  to  many  years  of  cross  breeding 
with  abandoned,  lost  or  stolen  domestic  breeds  is,  in  most 
cases,  little  different  than  any  other  domestic  light  horse 
(Beebe  and  Johnson,  1964).   Ryden  (1970)  states  that  there  are 
only  a  few  hundred  pure-blooded  descendants  of  the  original 
Spanish  horses  and  these  are  in  captivity  in  special  registers 
in  North  and  South  America.   She  also  states  that  the  Spanish 
horse's  traits  often  appear  in  animals  that  have  as  little  as 
3.0  percent  Spanish  ancestry. 

The  significance  of  the  number  of  lumbar  vertebrae  to  determine 
whether  a  horse  has  Spanish  ancestry  or  not  is  open  to  question. 
Stecher  (1962)  studied  the  lateral  joints  in  the  caudal  lumbar 
region  of  horses.   He  examined  the  skeletal  remains  of  245 
horses  of  nine  species  obtained  from  various  sources.   The 
horses  were  classified  according  to  whether  they  had  five  or 
six  lumbar  vertebrae.   Skeletal  specimens  included  the  domestic 
horse,  the  arabian,  the  Shetland  pony,  Przewalski's  horse,  the 
hybrid  mule,  two  species  of  asses  and  two  of  zebras.   The  Shet- 
land pony  was  the  only  species  in  which  all  samples  (eight) 


contained  six  lumbar  vertebrae;  the  Hemoine  (Equus  hemoinus) , 
the  Mongolian  wild  ass,  was  the  only  species  in  which  all 
samples  (nine)  contained  five  lumbar  vertebrae.   Skeletons  of 
ten  arabians  showed  three  with  five  and  seven  with  six  verte- 
brae.  Two  domestic  horses  had  five  and  one  half,  and  one 
Grevy's  zebra  (Equus  grevyi)  had  seven  lumbar  vertebrae. 

Edwards  (1970)  states  that  arabian  horses,  regardless  of  their 
purity,  do  not  always  have  five  lumbar  vertebrae  any  more  than 
Przewalski's  horse  always  had  five  or  six.  It  depended  on  who 
you  were  talking  to. 

Howell  (1945)  wrote  that  the  American  Museum  of  Natural  History 
stated  in  a  letter  to  him  that  the  proper  number  of  lumbar 
vertebrae  for  the  arabian  is  five  while  that  of  other  horses  is 
six.   The  statement  was  qualified  by  stating  that  the  point 
which  constitutes  a  lumbar  vertebrae  is  its  transverse  processes 
whereas  a  dorsal  vertebrae  is  determined  by  the  attachments  of 
ribs  in  place  of  transverse  processes.   Therefore,  it  is  safer 
to  say  that  arab  horses  have  23  dorso-lumbars  while  other 
varieties  have  24. 

Feist  (1971)  and  Hall  (1972)  reported  skeletal  examples  of 
three  types  of  lumbar  vertebrae  in  the  Pryor  Mountain  horses. 
The  Barb  type  have  five  lumbar  vertebrae  and  17  pairs  of  ribs; 
the  Andulusian  have  five  and  a  half,  with  the  last  three  fused; 
and  the  modern  breeds  of  today  (except  the  Arabian)  have  six 
lumbar  vertebrae  with  no  fusion.   Appendix  2  depicts  three 
types  of  lumbar  vertebrae  found  in  equids. 

Colors.   Since  local  color  names  vary  from  one  geographical 
area  to  another,  it  is  difficult  to  portray  true  horse  colors 
verbally. 

The  original  Andulusian  horse  encompasses  the  entire  range  of 
horse  colors.   Spotting  occurs,  but  is  not  especially  character- 
istic (Simpson,  1951).   Smith  (1841)  wrote  that  all  South 
American  feral  horses  bore  the  stamp  of  the  domesticated  races 
of  old  Spain.   His  personal  observations  indicated  that  they 
were  mostly  of  similar  color  though  every  color  seen  in  Europe 
existed  among  them.   Grey,  or  shades  of  grey,  were  most  common 
in  the  northern  mountain  regions,  and  shades  of  bay  in  the 
pampas.   Black  was  the  rarest  color.   Hoyt  (1886),  an  old 
mustanger,  wrote  that  ninety  percent  of  the  feral  horses  on 
the  panhandle  of  Texas  were  bay,  dark  brown,  or  black,  and  that 
other  colors  were  rare.   Cook  (1919),  writing  in  1870,  stated 
that  on  the  plains  area  east  of  the  Rockies,  color  in  wild 
mustangs  consisted  of  cream,  buckskin  or  mouse  and  that  black 
stripes  above  the  knees  or  hocks  and  along  the  middle  of  the 
back  from  mane  to  tail  commonly  occurred.   Worcester  (1945,  p. 
416),  in  an  historical  review  of  Spanish  horses  among  the 


Indians,  stated  that  David  Thompson,  an  early  explorer,  commented 
on  hoof  color  in  Indian  horses:   "The  yellow  hoof  with  white 
hair  is  a  brittle  hoof  and  soon  wears  away;  for  this  reason,  as 
much  as  possible,  the  natives  take  only  black-hoofed  horses  on 
their  war  expeditions." 

Gremmel  (1939),  writing  in  the  Journal  of  Heredity,  stresses 
physical  differences  in  horse  coat  colors  from  an  histological 
point  of  view.   He  lists  the  basic  colors  and  the  patterns  that 
occur  within  these  basics  (see  Appendix  3). 

Dobie  (1952)  devoted  a  chapter  to  the  dun  color  in  wild  horses 
(p.  299): 

The  dun  and  the  stripe  are  always  waiting  to  come 
back.   All  colors  but  gray  and  roan  are,  in  bio- 
logical language,  recessive  to  dun.   I  cannot  draw  up 
tables  of  dates  and  numbers,  but  to  me  it  is  evident 
that  for  three  and  a  half  centuries,  say  from  1520  to 
1880,  dun  was  progressively  emerging  over  the  American 
continents  among  descendents  of  horses  marked  sparsely 
by  that  color  when  Spaniards  planted  them.   Before  that 
time  selective  breeding  had  driven  the  dun  color  into 
hiding;  feral  life  brought  it  back. 

There  is  a  wild  coat  pattern  gene  in  horses.   Przewalski's 
horse  is  not  uniform  black  but  has  a  distinctly  concealing 
color.   The  general  body  color  is  a  neutral  gray  but  peripheral 
areas,  including  mane,  tail,  dorsal  stripe  and  legs,  are  black. 
The  primitive  type  coat  color  depends  upon  four  dominant  genes. 
Recessive  mutations  have  modified  each  of  them.   However,  they 
form  the  basis  of  domestic  color  varieties,  many  of  which  would 
be  unable  to  survive  in  wild  populations  because  they  lack 
concealing  value  (Castle,  1954).   Feist  (1971)  listed  eight 
basic  colors  for  the  Pryor  Mountain  horses.   Variations  to  these 
basics  that  appear  on  legs,  manes  or  tails  were  classified  as 
secondary  markings.   Hall  (1972,  p.  19)  noted  that  the  Pryor 
Mountain  herd  seemed  to  be  reverting  back  to  the  original 
mustang  type.  Among  other  characteristics  he  refers  to  color. 
"The  reversion  of  colors  to  the  blue  corn,  buckskins,  and  bays 
with  line  stripes  down  their  back,  along  their  withers  and  on 
their  legs." 

There  are  many  reports  that  stallions  often  show  color  prefer- 
ences for  the  mares  they  select  for  their  harems  and  will  reject 
those  whose  color  does  not  meet  their  requirements  (Dobie,  1952; 
Feist,  1971;  Ryden,  1970).   Since  color  vision  has  not  been 
scientifically  established  in  horses  (Hafez,  e_t  al,  1969; 
Smythe,  1966),  the  selection  of  certain  colored  mares  may  be 
based  on  brightness  or  intensity  rather  than  hue. 


Size  of  horses.   There  are  no  extensive  records  of  wild 
horse  sizes  or  weights.   The  original  Spanish  horse  was  small, 
rarely  attaining  15  hands.   Some  of  the  Indian  mounts  reached 
only  12  to  13  hands.   Present-day  stallions  seldom  exceed  1000 
pounds,  and  mares  may  be  as  light  as  700  pounds  (Ryden,  1970). 
In  the  Pryor  Mountain  herd,  females  averaged  600  to  750  pounds 
and  males,  800  to  850  pounds  (Hall,  1972).   Cook  (1919),  who 
wrote  of  the  period  from  1870  to  1880,  estimated  that  the 
average  wild  horse  weighed  about  800  pounds.   Schwartz  (1949) 
reported  that  wild  horses  sold  to  fox  ranches  and  other  outlets 
that  buy  horses  for  slaughter  averaged  between  750  and  850 
pounds  but  some  of  them  weighed  as  much  as  1250  pounds.   Horses 
from  the  North  often  weighed  more  than  those  from  the  South  due 
to  more  draft  blood  from  escapees  of  the  World  War  I  period; 
however,  the  horses  in  certain  parts  of  Oregon  were  small, 
averaging  800  pounds  in  weight.   The  Fort  Apache  herds,  which 
had  as  high  a  percentage  of  wild  mustang  blood  as  any,  averaged 
about  650  to  700  pounds  (Wyman,  1945). 

A  factor  affecting  the  growth  of  wild  horses,  and  one  of  the 
reasons  many  of  them  remain  smaller  than  domestic  horses,  is 
their  restricted  diet.   They  rarely  eat  grain  like  domestic 
horses,  and  their  diet  is  often  deficient  in  minerals  and 
vitamins,  plus  the  fact  that  salt  and  adequate  water  are  not 
always  available  to  them  (Beebe  and  Johnson,  1964).   Other 
writers  also  mention  inadequate  and  poor  quality  of  food  as  a 
reason  for  the  small  size  of  wild  horses  (Ensminger,  1951;  Hall, 
1972;  Ryden,  1970). 

The  maturation  rate  in  wild  horses  is  slower  than  in  domestic 
horses.   Two-  and  three-year-old  horses  often  appear  to  be 
yearlings  (Ryden,  1970;  Hall,  1972). 

Length  of  life  in  wild  horses.   There  is  little  data  on 
the  lifespan  of  horses  in  the  wild  state.   Domestic  horses 
that  reach  their  twenties  are  considered  old;  however,  it  is 
not  uncommon  for  horses  to  reach  their  thirties  and  some  have 
been  recorded  that  survived  past  forty  years  of  age  (Simpson, 
1951).   The  maximum  life  span  of  horses  in  the  Pryor  Mountains 
is  ten  to  fourteen  years  and  their  maximum  breeding  period  is 
five  to  six  years  (Hall,  1972).   Dobie  (1952)  relates  that 
Black  Kettle,  a  famous  and  legendary  wild  horse  that  was  cap- 
tured past  his  prime,  lived  to  about  thirty  years  of  age. 

Aging  horses.   The  age  of  horses  is  generally  determined 
by  the  amount  of  wear  of  their  teeth.   The  mature  male  has  forty 
teeth;  a  young  animal,  male  or  female,  has  twenty- four  teeth. 
The  mature  female  lacks  tushes  and  therefore  has  thirty-six 
teeth.   In  horses  up  to  five  years  of  age  the  numbers  of  perma- 
nent and  milk  teeth  are  noted.   From  six  to  twelve  years,  age 
is  estimated  by  the  number  of  cups  or  indentations  in  the 


incisor  teeth.  After  twelve  years,  age  may  be  judged  by  the 
cross  section  and  slant  of  the  incisors.   Learning  to  determine 
age  in  horses  is  a  matter  of  practical  experience.   When  a 
horse  exceeds  twelve  years,  even  the  most  experienced  investi- 
gators have  difficulty  in  determining  accurate  age  (Ensminger, 
1951).  Horses  that  live  in  areas  of  sandy  or  gritty  soils 
show  increased  dental  wear.   Under  these  conditions  a  six- year- 
old  horse  may  appear  to  be  ten  years  old  (Bone,  1964). 

Locomotion.   It  is  only  since  the  development  of  the  camera 
that  man  has  learned  accurately  how  a  horse  coordinates  its 
four  legs  during  execution  of  the  various  speeds  of  movement. 

Horsemen  recognize  as  many  as  twelve  gaits  in  horses,  but  these 
are  all  variations  of  the  walk,  trot,  pace  and  gallop.   The  pace 
is  included  because  a  few  horses  pace  naturally.   The  canter, 
which  is  a  slow  gallop,  usually  has  to  be  taught.   The  lope, 
recognized  by  western  horsemen,  is  a  smooth  gallop  or  canter 
which  sometimes  verges  on  a  four-beat  rhythm  similar  to  a  run- 
ning walk. 

The  walk  has  a  four-beat  cadence  with  the  succession  of  foot- 
falls being  left-fore,  right-hind,  right-fore,  left-hind.   The 
body  is  alternately  supported  on  three  and  on  two  legs.   The 
trot  is  a  faster  gait  with  a  two-beat  cadence  where  the  diagonal 
legs  move  together  so  that  the  sequence  of  feet  striking  the 
ground  is  left- fore,  right-hind  followed  by  right-fore,  left- 
hind.   Twice  in  each  stride  there  is  an  interval  when  all  four 
feet  are  off  the  ground.   The  pace  has  similar  rhythm  to  the 
trot  but  the  two  legs  of  the  same  side,  not  the  diagonals,  move 
together.   It  is  a  slow,  broken  pace  tending  toward  a  walk. 

The  gallop  is  the  horse's  fastest  gait  and  differs  from  the 
others  in  not  being  symmetrical  on  the  two  sides.   It  may  be 
led  either  on  one  side  or  the  other.   The  cadence  is  three 
beat,  with  a  broken  rhythm  that  occurs  as  beat,  pause,  beat, 
quick  double  beat,  beat,  pause,  etc.   The  pause  represents  the 
short  period  when  all  four  feet  are  off  the  ground,  which 
occurs  only  once  in  each  stride.   The  sequence  of  footfall  for 
the  left  lead  is  left-fore,  right-hind,  left-hind  and  right-fore, 
the  latter  two  almost  together  with  the  hind  foot  slightly 
ahead.   The  right-hind  foot  is  lifted  almost  immediately  as  the 
left  hind  foot  comes  down  and  before  the  right  fore  foot  strikes. 
Normally,  at  no  time  are  three  of  the  horse's  feet  on  the 
ground.   The  right  lead  is  a  mirror  image  of  the  left  lead.   In 
an  extended  gallop  there  may  be  four  beats,  as  opposed  hind 
and  fore  feet  strike  at  perceptible  intervals;  however,  the 
break  after  the  placing  of  the  lead  foot  still  distinguishes 
the  cadence.   Horses  can  be  taught  to  lead  either  right  or  left 
or  to  change  from  one  lead  to  the  other  while  galloping.   This 
is  important  if  the  animal  is  to  turn  rapidly,  because  a  horse 


making  a  sharp  left  turn  on  the  right  lead  is  liable  to  fall 
(Simpson,  1951;  Tricker  and  Tricker,  1966). 

Few  horses  jump  regularly  unless  they  are  taught.   This  may  be 
due  in  part  to  their  lack  of  binocular  vision  which  prevents 
them  from  judging  the  correct  take-off  distances  (Hafez  et  al, 
1969). 

Drawings  from  a  motion  picture  strip  (Appendix  4)  depict  leg 
positions  of  the  horse  while  walking,  trotting  and  galloping. 


Population  Characteristics 

Some  of  the  properties  of  a  collective  group  of  organisms  or 
populations  are  density,  birth  rate,  death  rate,  age  distri- 
bution, biotic  potential,  dispersion  and  growth  form.   Genetic 
characteristics  such  as  adaptiveness ,  reproductive  fitness,  and 
persistence  (leaving  descendants  over  long  periods  of  time)  are 
also  directly  related  to  populations  (Odum,  1971). 

There  are  three  major  age  groups  in  a  population,  the  pre- 
reproductive,  the  reproductive  and  the  post- reproductive.   In 
a  rapidly  growing  population,  growth  may  be  exponential  due  to 
a  high  birthrate  and  each  successive  generation  will  be  more 
numerous.   This  results  graphically  in  a  pyramid  age  structure. 
The  graphic  representation  of  a  stable  population  is  bell  shaped, 
The  pre-reproductive  and  reproductive  age  groups  are  fairly 
equal  in  size  and  the  post- reproductive  age  group  remains  small. 
If  the  birth  rate  is  drastically  reduced  the  reproductive, 
post- reproductive  age  groups  increase  proportionally  which 
results  in  an  urn-shaped  graph  representative  of  a  declining 
population  (Boughey,  1968). 

Odum  (1971)  describes  characteristics  of  populations  in  regard 
to  age  structure.   An  expanding  population  contains  a  large 
proportion  of  young  animals;  a  stationary  population  a  more 
even  distribution  of  age  classes;  a  declining  population  a  large 
proportion  of  old  individuals. 

The  population  density  of  wild  horses,  like  that  of  any  other 
animals,  must  be  in  tune  with  and  not  exceed  the  carrying 
capacity  of  the  available  habitat.   Other  than  this  general 
observation  and  the  limited  data  that  Hall  (1972)  and  Feist 
(1971)  collected  on  the  Pryor  Mountain  horses,  there  is  no 
scientific  information  available  on  optimum  population  levels, 
the  results  of  overcrowding,  competition,  or  the  effects  of  a 
degrading  habitat.   However,  there  is  evidence  that  wild  horse 
densities  could  be  reduced,  through  improper  management  and 
control,  to  a  level  that  could  lead  to  their  extinction  either 
locally  or  nationally. 


There  is  a  critical  population  size  that  varies  from  species 
to  species.   Once  the  population  density  of  a  particular  species 
goes  below  this  level  it  is  doomed  to  extinction  regardless  of 
efforts  to  save  it.   Prime  examples  are  the  passenger  pigeon 
which  died  out  completely  even  though  hunting  had  ceased  and 
there  were  still  several  thousand  remaining  birds  scattered 
over  North  America;  the  Heath  hen,  although  rigorously  protected 
(after  the  population  level  had  become  small) ,  suffered  the 
same  fate. 

Since  their  social  life  plays  an  important  part  in  locating 
feeding  areas,  raising  their  young  and  defending  against 
enemies,  gregarious  animals  such  as  the  ungulates  are  particu- 
larly susceptible  to  the  danger  of  extinction  when  their  density 
reaches  a  certain  level.   A  number  of  factors  may  determine 
this  critical  population  size.   Three  of  the  more  important  of 
these  are:   (1)  males  fail  to  find  females  due  to  low  density, 
(2)  courtship  behavior  is  inhibited  by  local  low  population 
density,  (3)  the  remaining  population  is  too  small  to  resist 
predators  and  competitors  (Ziswiler,  1967).   Erhenfeld  (1972) 
adds  another  factor:   the  population  size  may  be  too  small  for 
reproduction  to  compensate  for  losses  from  disease,  climatic 
conditions,  or  natural  disasters. 


Distribution  of  Wild  Horses 

The  public  land  administered  by  the  Bureau  of  Land  Management 
and  the  U.  S.  Forest  Service  that  contain  habitat  for  wild 
horses  are  located  in  Arizona,  California,  Colorado,  Idaho, 
Montana,  Nevada,  New  Mexico,  Utah  and  Wyoming.   The  largest 
concentrations  of  wild  horses  are  in  Nevada,  Wyoming  and  Oregon. 

Their  distribution  is  generally  limited  to  areas  where  accessi- 
bility is  limited,  population  of  humans  is  sparse,  and  the 
terrain  rugged.   Their  range  is  also  limited  by  availability 
of  forage,  water  and  the  numbers  of  fences  or  barriers  that 
restrict  movement.   The  total  extent  of  wild  horse  habitat  on 
lands  not  federally  administered  is  not  known.   Thousands  of 
acres  of  these  lands  are  not  fenced  and  the  wild  horses  have 
access  to  them. 

Populations  of  wild  horses  in  the  grassland  biome  are  small,  a 
few  have  been  reported  in  New  Mexico  and  in  Montana.   The 
greatest  numbers  inhabit  the  cold  desert  and  the  pinon- juniper 
type  of  the  woodland-brushland  biome.   Seasonally,  in  some  areas 
they  range  into  the  montane  coniferous  forest  biome.   In  general 
this  biome  is  too  cold  in  the  winter  for  the  horses  to  remain 
there  year  round.   See  map,  appendix  8-. 


Population  Status 

The  proceedings  of  the  first  National  Advisory  Board  meeting 
for  wild  horses  and  burros  (January  1973)  reported  an  estimated 
total  of  2000  horses  on  Forest  Service  controlled  lands  and 
16,878  horses  on  Bureau  of  Land  Management  controlled  lands. 

Both  agencies  state  that  1975  population  estimates,  as  presented 
below,  are  of  much  higher  quality  than  their  earlier  data  due 
primarily  to  more  intensive  efforts  and  improved  methods  of 
censusing.   Bureau  of  Land  Management  controlled  lands  are 
listed  by  state;  Forest  Service  lands  by  forest  region.   Data 
on  state,  private  and  other  lands  is  not  available.   (See  map, 
Appendix  8) . 

Bureau  of  Land  Management  Wild  Horse  Inventory  Data  as  of  May  1,  1975 
(Prepared  by  Milton  Frei,  U.S.  Bureau  of  Land  Mgt.,  Denver,  Colo.) 

Estimates  of  Population 

Animals  Claimed 

4 
321 

14 

110 

6,463 

5,268 

1,209 

1 

725 


State 

Horses 

Arizona 

109 

California 

3,373 

Colorado 

697 

Idaho 

865 

Montana 

314 

New  Mexico 

6,543 

Nevada 

21,868 

Oregon 

6,928 

Utah 

1,670 

Wyoming 

7,291 

49,658*       14,115 

*  Includes  animals  claimed,  proof  of  ownership 
still  has  to  be  determined  under  Section  5  of 
the  1971  Wild  Horse  and  Burro  Act 


National  Forest  Wild  Free-Roaming  Horse  Inventory  Data  (Estimate) 
(Adapted  from  a  letter  dated  April  15,  1975,  from  the  Assistant 
Director  for  Environmental  Coordination,  U.S.  Forest  Service) 

No.  of 
Population       Animals 
Region      National  Forest       as  of  1-1-75     Claimed 

1         Custer  8 


Region  Total 

8 

Shoshone-Bridger 

7 

Region  Total 

7 

Apache- Si tgreaves 

Carson 

Gila 

Santa  Fe 

7 

161 

6 

60 

Region  Total        234 


Challis 

Payette 

Dixie 

Wasatch 

Humboldt 

Toiyabe 


3 

13 

13 

60 

20 

485 

812 

16 

1,393 

29 

22 

50 

500 

9 

298 

Region  Total 

Klamath 

Lassen 

Modoc 

Los  Padres 

Inyo 

Region  Total        879 

Malheur  174         44 

Ochoco  60  1 

Region  Total        234  45 

GRAND  TOTAL    2,755  74 


10 


Origin  and  History  of  Wild  Horses 

Origin.   Present-day  horses  of  all  kinds  have  descended  from 
a  small,  four-toed,  rodent-like  creature  whose  name  many  people 
are  familiar  with,  eohippus,  or  the  dawn  horse.   The  correct 
scientific  name,  however,  is  Hyracotherium.   This  came  about 
because  early  scientists  (1838)  did  not  recognize  that  the 
fossil  remains  of  a  small  animal  found  near  Suffolk,  England 
was  related  to  horses,  and  compared  the  remains  to  the  Hyraxes, 
which  the  fossils  closely  resembled.   Hyraxes  are  comparable 
in  size  and  external  appearance  to  rodents  and  lagomorphs.   When 
similar  fossils  were  found  in  North  America  at  a  later  date  the 
principle  of  evolution  had  become  well  established  and  they 
were  recognized  as  horse  ancestors.   Charles  Marsh  of  Yale 
University  gave  them  the  euphonius  name,  eohippus.   Since 
Hyracotherium  is  much  the  older  of  the  two  names,  under  the 
rules  of  zoological  nomenclature  it  is  the  correct  one  to  use. 

Eohippus  lived  at  the  same  time  in  both  Europe  and  North  America, 
appearing  in  both  places  at  the  very  beginning  of  the  Eocene. 
No  direct  ancestors  of  eohippus  have  been  found  on  either 
continent.   There  were  several  species  of  eohippus  which  varied 
greatly  in  size,  the  smallest  being  not  much  over  10  inches  in 
height  at  the  shoulder,  the  largest  about  20  inches.   Four  toes 
occurred  on  the  front  foot  of  eohippus,  each  ending  in  a 
separate  small  hoof.   The  hind  foot  had  only  three  functional 
toes.   The  animal  was  already  distinctly  herbivorous,  with 
teeth  modified  for  browsing  rather  than  grazing  (Simpson,  1951). 

In  the  beginning  of  the  Cenozoic  Era  of  geologic  time  (70  to  75 
million  years  ago)  the  British  Isles  and  North  America  were 
attached  as  part  of  the  supercontinent  of  Laurasia,  which  also 
included  Greenland  and  Europe  north  of  the  Alps  and  east  to  the 
Himalayas.   During  the  long  period  when  Laurasia  was  separating, 
animals  were  able  to  migrate  back  and  forth  on  the  land  bridges 
that  still  connected  Europe  and  North  America.   During  this  time 
eohippus  evolved  and  occupied  both  North  America  and  Europe. 
Not  long  after  this  the  continents  separated  and  migration 
ceased.   In  Europe  for  an  unknown  reason  eohippus  became  extinct. 
In  North  America  through  a  period  of  about  fifty  million  years 
eohippus  evolved  into  Mercyhippus ,  an  animal  with  high  crowned 
teeth  which  permitted  it  to  graze  rather  than  browse.   It  was 
also  larger,  about  the  size  of  a  small  pony  of  today. 

After  Mercyhippus  there  is  a  gap  in  the  fossil  records  from 
about  six  million  years  ago  to  about  600,000  B.C.,  during  which 
time  Mercyhippus  evolved  into  Equus  caballus ,  the  true  horse, 
having  a  single  toe  on  each  foot.   No  fossil  remains  of  horses 
have  been  found  in  North  America  for  the  pleistocene  epoch,  the 
Great  Ice  Age.   The  scanty  records  that  were  available  came 
mostly  from  Eurasia.   Perhaps  when  the  ice  melted,  fossil  records 


11 


were  washed  away  in  the  huge  floods  that  followed.   Equus 
caballus  may  have  evolved  from  some  of  its  North  American 
ancestors  that  crossed  the  land  bridge  which  connected  Alaska 
and  Siberia  during  this  period.   Changes  in  conditions  which 
are  still  unclear  brought  the  horse  back  to  North  America  about 
600,000  B.C.   Fossil  records  dating  from  this  time  to  about 
7000  B.C.  have  been  found  in  many  places  on  the  North  American 
continent.   After  7000  B.C.,  horses  again  vanished  along  with 
several  other  species  of  large  grasseaters.   There  was  no  such 
extinction  of  species  in  Europe.   As  the  ice  sheet  melted, 
Europe  warmed  and  the  forests  encroached  on  the  grasslands. 
Grazing  herds  were  forced  eastward  onto  the  steppes  of  southern 
Russia  and  western  Asia.   Tribes  of  early  hunters  followed  the 
herds,  became  semi-nomadic  and  by  5000  B.C.  had  hunting  dogs 
and  had  domesticated  the  onager  (Asian  wild  ass)  and  reindeer 
(Simpson,  1951;  Haines,  1971).   Appendix  5  depicts  features  of 
horse  evolution. 

Domestication  of  the  horse.   By  about  4000  B.C.  two,  and 
possibly  three,  species  of  E.  caballus  had  developed  in  the 
Eurasian  grasslands.   Around  the  Black  and  Caspian  Seas  the 
"Tarpan"  and  in  Mongolia  or  Manchuria  Przewalski's  horse  had 
developed.   A  third  subspecies,  depicted  as  a  forest  horse,  was 
larger  than  the  other  two  and  existed  in  Polish  forests  until 
the  middle  of  the  eighteenth  century.   Disagreement  exists  as 
to  whether  it  was  a  true  subspecies. 

Truly  wild  horses  were  common  from  Europe  through  central  Asia 
in  early  historic  times.   The  European  breed  called  "Tarpan"  is 
now  extinct  and  it  is  uncertain  if  Przewalski's  horse  survives 
in  pure  form.   There  is  general  agreement,  however,  that  the 
"Tarpan"  was  the  first  domesticated  horse. 

Later,  Indo-Europeans  invaded  the  region  from  the  southeast  and 
possibly  tamed  a  few  horses.   By  4000  B.C.  they  were  using 
horses  to  pull  carts.   Horse  culture  spread  north  and  east  into 
Mongolia  and  turned  many  tribes  into  nomads.   About  2000  B.C. 
tribesmen  from  the  Asian  steppes  crossed  the  Iranian  plateau 
with  horses  and  overran  the  entire  near  east.   A  thousand  years 
later  horses  appeared  in  North  Africa,  west  to  Gibraltar,  and 
north  and  west  through  Europe  as  far  as  Scandinavia,  Spain, 
France,  and  the  British  Isles.   The  earliest  records  of  horses 
in  Greece  appear  about  1700  B.C.,  in  Egypt,  1600  B.C.  and  in  India, 
1500  B.C.   By  1000  B.C.,  and  possibly  earlier,  they  had  reached 
Spain.   The  first  horses  that  were  imported  from  north  of  the 
Caucasus  were  small,  stocky  animals,  more  commonly  used  for 
pulling  carts  and  chariots  than  riding.   Later  the  Egyptians 
involved  in  trading  and  breeding  horses  crossed  these  with  the 
more  fleet  horses  that  came  from  the  Libyans  or  Numidians  of 
North  Africa. 


12 


There  are  several  theories  concerning  the  development  of  modern 
horses.   One  of  the  most  popular  at  present  is  that  all  the 
light,  fast,  Mediterranean  and  mideastern  horses  arose  from  a 
single  original  stock  now  represented  by  the  Arabian.   If  this 
is  true,  the  Numidian  horses  now  represented  by  the  Barb  (named 
from  Barbary,  an  old  African  country  west  of  Libya)  were  of 
common  origin  with  the  Arabian.   The  Barb  and  Arabian,  however, 
as  far  back  as  they  can  be  traced,  are  of  distinct  types.   They 
are  both  light  and  fast,  but  the  former  lacks  the  wedge-shaped 
head,  the  dished  profile  and  protruding  eyes  of  the  Arabian. 
Both  supposedly  have  23  lumbar  vertebrae,  whereas  24  is  the 
usual  number  in  all  other  breeds  . 

A  heavy  horse  capable  of  carrying  armored  knights  was  first 
developed  in  Germany,  France  and  the  British  Isles.   The  early 
horse  of  Spain  was  a  heavy  breed  similar  to  those  used  in 
France  and  Germany.   When  the  Moors  invaded  and  conquered  Spain, 
the  Spaniards  recognized  the  superiority  of  Moor  horses  which 
were  mostly  Barb  with  perhaps  some  mix  of  Arabian.   They  crossed 
their  own  Norse  Dun  breed  with  the  Barb-Arabian  mixture.   The 
product,  called  the  Jennet,  was  so  superior  that  for  the  next 
several  centuries  Spain  was  renowned  for  the  quality  of  its 
horses,  especially  those  from  the  Andulusia  area  of  Spain. 

The  Andulusian  type  is  the  breed  that  was  first  brought  to 
the  new  world  by  Spanish  conquistadors.   Later  it  escaped  onto 
our  western  plains  and  the  pampas  of  South  America  and  became 
the  wild  horse  of  the  West  and  of  Argentina  (Simpson,  1951; 
Ryden,  1970;  Haines,  1971). 

Influence  of  horses  on  history.   Since  their  domestication, 
horses  have  had  a  profound  influence  on  history.   Perhaps  the 
most  significant  has  been  the  use  of  horses  in  warfare.   They 
have  been  involved  in  every  war  up  until  recent  times,  and  only 
within  the  present  century  has  the  development  of  horses  for 
military  use  ceased  to  be  the  main  concern  of  breeders. 

The  Mohammedan  and  Mongolian  conquests  were  made  possible  by 
horses.   In  the  seventeenth  century  the  Mohammedans  were  stopped 
by  the  Franks  only  because  the  Franks  were  clad  in  armor  and 
mounted  on  heavy,  strong  horses.   The  Mongolians  were  never 
really  stopped;  their  empire  collapsed  because  of  its  unwieldy 
size. 

Horses  since  ancient  time  have  been  important  sources  of  power 
for  agriculture  and  transportation,  for  recreation,  as  status 
symbols  or  emblems  of  wealth  and  authority,  for  food,  leather, 
and  other  products.   The  horse  is  still  essential  as  a  source 
of  rural  power  and  transportation  in  many  countries. 


13 


The  American  nation  was  built  on  horsepower  and  even  today,  when 
the  horse  is  no  longer  essential,  it  remains  a  part  of  our 
culture.   The  mechanized  age  lessened  the  importance  of  the 
horse  as  a  principle  factor  in  American  development,  but  it 
still  has  use  for  riding,  hunting,  recreation  and  as  an  animal 
that  people  like  to  be  associated  with  (Simpson,  1951). 

The  horse  in  America.   It  is  difficult  to  state  flatly  how 
or  where  horses  first  escaped  or  were  stolen  from  the  Spaniards 
and  reverted  to  a  wild  state.   On  Columbus'  second  voyage  in 
1493  he  landed  horses  from  Andulusia  in  the  West  Indies.   Ponce 
de  Leon  brought  horses  from  Cuba  or  Puerto  Rico  to  the  coast 
of  Florida  in  1521.   Cortez  carried  horses  with  him  when  he 
discovered  the  Aztec  civilization  in  1519.   Horses  were  intro- 
duced into  the  pampas  of  South  America  by  Pedro  Mendoza  in  1535. 
However,  it  is  likely  that  wild  horses  which  later  covered  the 
pampas  came  from  Chile  and  not  from  the  small  number  Mendoza 
abandoned.   During  the  early  years  of  exploration,  horses  were 
sent  in  nearly  every  ship  leaving  Spanish  ports.   The  Spaniards 
also  established  breeding  farms  in  the  West  Indian  colonies  of 
Cuba,  Puerto  Rico  and  Santo  Domingo,  and  stocked  them  with  the 
finest  stallions  and  brood  mares  that  had  been  brought  out  of 
Spain.   So  many  horses  were  transported  that  Spain  finally 
placed  an  embargo  on  the  animals  because  they  lacked  enough 
horses  for  home  use.   At  one  critical  point  horses  were  in  such 
short  supply  that  the  breeding  farms  in  the  West  Indies  were 
forced  to  import  more  Barbs  from  North  Africa,  thus  the  Barb 
was  once  again  introduced  into  the  line  (Simpson,  1951;  Ryden, 
1970;  Haines,  1971). 

Indian  tribes  in  Texas  and  New  Mexico  were  probably  the  first  to 
obtain  horses.   The  Apaches  and  Comanches  got  horses  by  raiding 
the  Spanish  camps;  they  sold  or  bartered  horses  to  other  tribes 
as  did  Spanish  and  French  traders.   Soon  horses  spread  to  the 
Navajos,  Zunis,  Utes,  and  other  tribes  in  the  Southwest  and 
thence  to  tribes  in  the  southern  plains  and  northern  plains. 
By  1750  even  the  Blackfoot  Indians  in  Canada  had  horses  (Smith, 
1969). 

There  are  no  reliable  estimates  of  the  number  of  feral  horses 
in  the  U.  S.  during  the  late  1700 's  to  early  1800' s.   This  was 
the  time  of  maximum  horse  population.   McKnight  (1959)  estimates 
anywhere  between  2  and  5  million.   The  greatest  numbers  occurred 
in  the  Southwest,  with  the  most  densely  populated  ranges  in 
west  central  Texas. 

The  introduction  of  barbed  wire  and  fencing  marked  the  end  of 
the  wild  horses  on  the  Great  Plains  and  significantly  reduced 
their  numbers  elsewhere.   At  the  end  of  the  19th  century  most 
wild  horse  concentrations  were  to  be  found  west  of  the  Rocky 
Mountains . 


14 


The  Boer  War  in  South  Africa  created  a  large  demand  for  horses. 
Thousands  of  wild  horses  were  caught  and  shipped  to  Africa,  and 
since  many  died  en  route  or  were  killed  in  action,  the  demand 
continued  until  the  war  ended  in  1902.   During  World  War  I, 
amateur  and  professional  horse  hunters  found  they  could  sell 
all  the  wild  horses  they  could  catch. 

In  the  early  1920' s  four  new  markets  developed  that  helped  fur- 
ther decrease  wild  horse  herds:   the  use  of  horse  meat  for 
chicken  feed,  pet  foods,  human  food  and  an  increase  in  horse 
use  by  southern  farmers  when  cotton  prices  dropped  and  tractors 
became  too  expensive.   The  demand  was  partly  supplied  by  trained 
mustangs  captured  from  wild  bands. 

The  blood  of  remaining  wild  herds  was  further  diluted  during 
the  depression  of  the  thirties.   Large  numbers  of  marginal 
farmers  and  ranchers  went  out  of  business.   These  early  settlers, 
unable  to  sell  their  domestic  horses,  released  or  abandoned 
them.   Many  of  these  joined  the  roving  bands  of  wild  horses. 

The  passage  of  the  Taylor  Grazing  Act  in  1934  also  affected 
wild  horses,  since  stockmen  became  reluctant  to  share  their 
assigned  grazing  areas  with  wild  horses.   As  a  result,  ranchers, 
professional  horse  catchers,  and  the  federal  agencies  cooperated 
on  drives  to  remove  the  horses.   When  this  Act  became  law  it 
was  estimated  that  there  were  some  150,000  wild  horses  remaining 
on  public  lands  in  the  eleven  western  states. 

World  War  II  brought  temporary  relief  but  after  the  war,  efforts 
to  rid  the  range  of  wild  horses  was  again  initiated.   During 
the  late  1940' s  and  50' s,  over  100,000  horses  were  removed  from 
Nevada  rangelands,  while  smaller  numbers  were  removed  from 
other  western  ranges  (Denhardt,  1948;  McKnight,  1959;  Smith, 
1969;  Ryden,  1970).   (See  historical  distribution  of  wild 
horses,  Appendix  7) 

The  wild  horse  controversy.   The  controversy  over  wild 
horses  simmered  for  many  years.   Public  concern  for  the  plight 
of  the  wild  horse  gradually  increased  as  people  became  aware 
that  the  methods  of  capture  were  barbarous,  the  treatment 
brutal  and  inhumane,  and  the  final  product,  pet  or  chicken  food. 
During  the  1920' s  chicken  feed  producers  in  California  persuaded 
the  railroads  to  ship  horses  as  "chicken  feed"  thus  eliminating 
the  need  for  humane  treatment  during  transit. 

One  of  the  results,  and  the  primary  one,  was  a  rash  of  articles 
and  stories  in  national  magazines  and  in  the  news  media  about 
wild  horses.   Millions  of  people  who  had  never  heard  of  wild 
horses  were  now  concerned  and  they  reacted  by  writing  letters 
to  their  elected  representatives  (McKnight,  1959). 


15 


Several  different  philosophies  concerning  wild  horses  emerged. 
One  group,  including  many  stockmen  and  trained  scientists  do 
not  consider  the  wild  horse  wild.   They  claim  it  is  a  mixture 
of  many  breeds  of  the  domestic  horse-- that  the  blood  of  the 
original  Spanish  horse  is  so  diluted  as  to  be  almost  nonexistent. 
They  also  consider  it  a  nuisance  and  a  pest.   Another  group 
favors  native  wildlife  and  where  there  is  competition  they  feel 
that  the  horse  should  be  removed  or  rigidly  controlled.   A 
very  large  number  of  people  view  the  wild  horse  as  a  symbol  of 
the  old  West  and  that  all  wild  horses  should  remain  wherever  they 
are.   A  more  moderate  group  leans  toward  multiple  use.   They 
feel  that  the  wild  lands  are  for  the  use  of  all  animals  and 
that  the  condition  of  the  land  and  vegetation  has  first  prior- 
ity but  that  there  is  a  place  for  the  wild  horse.   They  are 
strongly  opposed  to  brutality  or  inhumane  treatment  when  popu- 
lations have  to  be  controlled  (Crain,  1973;  McKnight,  1959). 

As  a  result  of  continued  public  concern,  Congress  has  passed 
two  federal  laws  to  protect  wild  horses.   Public  Law  86-234,  passed 
in  1959,  prohibits  pollution  of  water  holes  for  trapping  and  the 
use  of  aircraft  or  motorized  vehicles  to  capture  or  kill  wild 
horses.   Public  Law  92-195,  passed  in  1971,  places  wild  horses 
and  burros  roaming  on  national  resource  lands  under  the  juris- 
diction of  the  Secretaries  of  Interior  and  Agriculture  for 
protection,  management  and  control.   It  provides  a  penalty  for 
harassing,  capturing,  killing  or  selling  wild  horses  or  burros 
and  prohibits  the  processing  of  either  animal  into  any  commercial 
product.   It  also  provides  for  the  establishment  of  an  advisory 
board  to  make  recommendations  to  the  Secretaries  on  management 
and  protection  of  wild  horses  and  burros.   The  above  statute 
omits  national  parks  and  monuments. 

The  first  advisory  board,  composed  of  nine  members,  met  on  four 
occasions  during  their  initial  year,  1973.   Meetings  were  held 
in  Salt  Lake  City,  Utah;  Denver,  Colorado;  Billings,  Montana; 
and  Lake  Havasu  City,  Arizona.   All  of  the  meetings  were  open 
to  the  public.   The  members  of  the  first  advisory  board  are 
listed  below: 

Name  Address 

Dr.  Charles  Wayne  Cook       4800  Venturi  Lane 

Fort  Collins,  CO  80521 

Mrs.  Velma  Johnston  140  Greenstone  Dr. 

Reno,  NV  89503 

Mrs.  Paul  Twyne  629  River  Bend  Rd. 

Great  Falls,  VA  22066 


16 


Dr.    Roger  Hungerford 


University  of  Arizona 
Tucson,  AZ   85721 


Mr.  Ed  Pierson 


Mr.  Ben  Glading 


Dr.  Floyd  W.  Frank 


Mr.  Roy  Young 


Mr.  Dean  Prosser,  Jr, 


Box  E 

Laporte,  CO  85035 

1413  El  Tejon  Way 
Sacramento,  CA  95826 

1395  Walenta  Way 
Moscow,  ID  83843 

P.  0.  Box  588 
Elko,  NV  89801 

P.  0.  Box  206 
Cheyenne,  WY  82001 


Reproduction 

Sexual  behavior  in  females.   During  diestrous,  if  the 
stallion  attempts  to  mount,  the  mare  displays  defensive  reactions 
varying  from  aggressiveness  to  disinterest.   As  estrous  approaches, 
the  mare  may  allow  the  stallion  to  smell  and  bite  her.   When 
ready  to  copulate,  the  tail  is  lifted  and  held  to  the  side,  the 
pelvis  lowered  and  the  hind  legs  spread.   The  intensity  of 
estrous  behavior  varies  between  individual  mares  and  peaks  just 
before  ovulation  (Hafez  et  al,  1969).   The  behavior  of  adult 
pony  mares  in  full  estrous  was  usually  passive  after  a  stallion 
had  shown  interest.   They  stand  quietly  with  hind  legs  straddled, 
tail  raised  and  often  turn  their  head  to  touch  the  stallion's 
muzzle.   On  occasions  they  squeal,  urinate  in  small  amounts  or 
paw  the  air  with  the  forefeet  (Tyler,  1972). 

Domestic  mares  generally  show  estrous  at  15  to  24  months  of  age. 
The  estrous  periods  recur  at  approximate  21-day  intervals  within 
a  range  of  18  to  27  days.   The  length  of  the  estrous  period 
varies  from  5  to  8  days  but  may  be  longer  in  early  spring.   The 
average  gestation  period  is  340  days  plus  or  minus  30  days. 
Postpartum  estrous  usually  occurs  seven  to  eleven  days  after 
foaling  (Ensminger,  1951). 

Under  domestication  the  average  conception  rate  of  horses  is 
less  than  50  percent.   In  the  wild  state,  when  horses  had  access 
to  good  forage  and  water  it  was  not  uncommon  for  the  conception 
rate  to  be  as  high  as  90  percent  (Ensminger,  1951).   The  method 
of  determining  the  conception  rate  for  wild  horses  was  not 
reported. 


17 


Within  the  body  of  literature  on  horses,  there  is  inconsistency 
pertaining  to  the  nature  and  length  of  the  breeding  seasons. 
The  terms  "polyestrous"  and  "breeding  season"  are  often  mis- 
interpreted. Mares  can  be  classified  into  three  categories, 
monoestrous,  true  polyestrous  and  transitory  polyestrous.   Wild 
breeds  are  monoestrous.   They  exhibit  several  estrous  cycles 
during  a  restricted  breeding  season  that  coincides  with  the 
longest  days  of  the  year.   Wild  mares  may  show  sexual  receptivity 
throughout  the  year  but  they  do  not  necessarily  breed  all  year 
round  (Berliner,  1958;  Hafez  et  al,  1969).   Clegg  and  Ganong 
(1969)  report  that  domestic  mares  will  breed  at  any  season  if 
they  are  well  fed.   When  mares  are  maintained  on  grass  they 
frequently  show  anestrous  during  the  winter.   They  suggest 
that  nutritional  factors  are  partly  responsible.   The  mare  is 
predominantly  a  spring  breeder;  therefore  she  would  be  expected 
to  respond  to  increasing  rather  than  decreasing  day  length 
(Clegg  and  Ganong,  1969).   A  wild  mare  will  go  barren  until 
the  following  year  if  she  is  not  bred  in  the  spring  or  early 
summer  (Feist,  1971). 

Burkhardt  (1947)  studied  the  influence  of  light  on  the  repro- 
ductive activity  of  domestic  mares.   He  divided  anestrous  mares 
into  four  groups.   One  group  received  artificial  illumination 
to  increase  day  length.  Another  group  was  exposed  to  ultra- 
violet light  applied  to  the  flank  and  belly.   The  mares  in  this 
latter  group  had  their  eyes  hooded.   Estrous  appeared  in  the 
group  receiving  extended  day  length  about  30  days  earlier  than 
the  control.   The  estrous  cycle  of  those  receiving  light  on  the 
flank  and  belly  was  normal.   The  author  suggested  that  since 
irradiation  of  the  ovaries  did  not  alter  the  estrous  cycle,  the 
receptor  organ  was  probably  the  eye.   Nishikawa  et  a_l  (1952) 
exposed  anestrous  mares  to  increased  day  length  from  mid- 
November  until  the  end  of  February.   They  concluded  that  light 
was  a  factor  but  not  the  only  one  controlling  reproductive 
activity  in  mares. 

Hall  (1972)  says  that  the  majority  of  mares  in  the  Pryor  Mountain 
herd  do  not  enter  estrous  until  they  are  three  years  old.   He 
does  not  think  that  these  horses  have  adjusted  their  estrous 
cycles  to  the  optimum  period  for  foal  survival.   He  relates 
reproductivity  to  nutrition  since  the  first  foals  (1971  and 
1972)  were  observed  in  April  and  the  majority  of  the  mares  were 
through  foaling  by  mid-July.   When  the  horses  have  been  on 
green  spring  forage  a  sufficient  time  for  their  nutritional 
state  to  improve  they  enter  estrous.   Their  reproductive  period 
starts  at  about  four  and  ends  at  about  nine  years  of  age. 
Tyler  (1972)  reported  that  most  pony  mares  foaled  for  the  first 
time  when  three  or  four  years  old  but  some  not  until  they  were 
five  years  old.   Very  few  of  the  141  mares  that  were  3  years 
old  or  more  foaled  in  each  of  the  3  years  of  study.   Foaling 
in  alternate  years  was  common;  so  were  abortions  during  the  late 
autumn,  winter  and  early  spring. 

18 


Sexual  behavior  in  the  stallion.   There  are  three  distinct 
phases  of  sexual  behavior  in  the  stallion:   courtship,  erection 
and  mounting,  and  intromission  and  ejaculation.   Sex  drive  is 
manifested  throughout  the  year.   Courtship  is  important  for 
successful  mating  because  the  stallion  depends  upon  erotic 
stimuli  to  achieve  vascular  engorgement  of  the  penis.   This  is 
elicited  by  visual,  auditory,  tactile,  and  olfactory  sensory 
modalities.   The  relative  importance  of  these  varies  among 
species  (Hafez  et  al,  1962).   During  courtship  the  stallion, 
after  smelling  the  mare,  exhibits  the  flehmen  posture  and  some- 
times snorts  or  whinnies  and  nibbles  or  licks  the  mare  before 
mounting  (Hafez  £t  al,  1969;  Feist,  1971;  Tyler,  1972).   True 
precopulatory  behavior  of  stallions  was  often  very  brief. 
Usually  it  was  longer  with  young  mares  than  with  older,  more 
experienced  mares.   The  older  mares  also  rejected  sexual  advances 
by  colts  and  bit  or  kicked  at  them  when  they  attempted  sniffing. 
Adult  stallions  chased  colts  away  from  adult  estrous  mares  but 
allowed  them  to  copulate  with  young  females  up  to  4  years  of 
age  (Tyler,  1972). 

Blakeslee  (1974)  was  told  by  the  owners  of  the  free-roaming 
appaloosa  horses  that  they  had  observed  only  one  instance  of  a 
foal  being  killed  by  a  stallion,  and  this  occurred  in  a  fenced 
enclosure.   Tyler  (1972)  stated  that  three  stallions  were 
probably  responsible  for  the  death  of  six  foals  and  the  injury 
of  several  others.   She  attributed  these  actions  to  frustration- 
induced  aggression  caused  by  mares  (who  were  not  in  full  estrous) 
rejecting  the  stallions'  sexual  advances. 

Proper  copulatory  patterns  are  apparent  in  many  stallions  at  10 
to  12  months  of  age.   However,  domestic  stallions  are  generally 
not  placed  in  service  until  three  years  of  age  (Enflminger,  1951; 
Hafez  et  al,  1962).   In  the  Pryor  Mountain  herd,  Hall  (1972) 
states  that  the  testes  of  stallions  do  not  descend  until  they  are 
three  years  of  age. 

Reproductive  activity  in  the  stallion,  like  the  mare,  is  also 
influenced  by  light.   Reduced  light  diminished  the  quantity 
and  quality  of  semen  whereas  increased  light  produced  an 
opposite  reaction  (Nishikawa  &t_  _al,  1952). 

Foaling.   With  normal  presentation  a  domestic  mare  foals  in 
fifteen  to  thirty  minutes.   The  foal  is  usually  born  while  the 
mare  is  lying  on  her  side  with  her  legs  stretched  out.   In 
normal  birth  the  front  feet  with  heels  down  appear  first, 
followed  by  the  nose  which  rests  on  the  front  legs.   The  hind 
legs  and  feet  are  last  (Ensminger,  1951). 

Blakeslee  (1974)  reported  that  she  was  advised  by  the  owners  of 
the  free- roaming  appaloosa  horses  that  any  birth  on  the  open 
range  lasting  much  longer  than  10  minutes  meant  that  the  mare 


19 


was  having  difficulty.   Tyler  (1972)  observed  a  pony  birth  that 
was  completed  within  25  minutes  from  the  time  the  mare  lay  down. 
Appaloosa  mares  generally  gave  birth  while  lying  in  an  upright 
position  with  the  forelegs  stretched  out  in  front  and  the  body 
weight  resting  on  the  sternum  (Blakeslee,  1974).   New  Forest 
ponies  delivered  while  lying  fully  recumbent  (Tyler,  1972). 

Blakeslee  (1974)  stated  that  appaloosa  mares  showed  no  preference 
for  a  birth  site  other  than  to  tend  to  avoid  the  night  grazing 
area  because  most  mares  foaled  at  night.   Birth  sites  with 
respect  to  vegetation  or  topography  were  not  determined  because 
all  observed  births  were  on  the  winter  range  which  was  flat  to 
rolling,  and  the  surrounding  vegetation  similar.   She  also 
reported  that  appaloosa  mares  almost  without  exception  isolated 
themselves  before  giving  birth,  and  the  dominant  mares  went 
greater  distances  from  the  group  than  the  subordinate  mares. 
Feist  (1971)  did  not  observe  any  births  within  the  Pryor  Mountain 
herd  but  did  find  foal  skeletons  in  secluded  areas.   Tyler  (1972) 
reported  that  pony  mares  chose  a  variety  of  birth  sites,  some- 
times secluded,  sometimes  near  their  group  companions  or  close 
to  busy  roads.   Dobie  (1952)  and  Ryden  (1971)  stated  that  the 
only  time  wild  stallions  permit  a  mare  to  leave  the  group  is  when 
she  is  about  to  foal. 

Dominant  appaloosa  mares  spent  longer  times  at  the  birth  site 
and  did  not  rejoin  their  family  groups  with  their  foals  as  soon 
as  subordinate  mares.   Mares  in  their  last  month  of  pregnancy 
or  geldings  sometimes  adopted  the  foals  of  primiparous  or 
subordinate  mares.   When  this  occurred  the  foal,  being  unable 
to  nurse,  usually  died.   Some  parturient  mares  permit  another 
mare  to  accompany  and  remain  near  them  at  the  birth  site.   This 
associate  is  nearly  always  a  mare  without  a  foal  of  her  own 
(Blakeslee,  1974). 

Reaction  of  other  horses  to  mating.   Tyler  (1972)  observed 
yearlings,  foals  and  sometimes  dominant  pony  mares  attempt  to 
prevent  mating  by  either  kicking  at  the  stud  or  estrous  mare  or 
by  placing  themselves  between  the  mare  and  stud.   Feist  (1971) 
observed  other  mares  kicking  at  both  the  estrous  mare  and  stud, 
and  Blakeslee  (1974)  observed  other  mares  kicking  at  the  stud. 
Neither  writer  reported  interference  by  foals  or  yearlings. 


Food  Habits 

Probably  the  most  important  change  that  occurred  in  horse 
evolution  was  the  transition  from  a  browsing  to  a  grazing 
animal.   The  teeth  of  early  horses  were  efficient  for  eating  a 
large  variety  of  foods  as  long  as  the  food  was  soft  and  did  not 
wear  down  the  teeth  too  rapidly.   Early  horses  could  not  have 
lived  on  grass  even  if  it  were  available  because  it  would  have 


20 


worn  out  their  teeth  at  an  early  age.   As  grasses  became  more 
abundant  (determined  from  fossil  grass  seeds)  many  browsing 
animals  unable  to  adapt,  disappeared.   The  horse  exploited  this 
change  by  evolving  teeth  that  permitted  them  to  change  from 
browsing  to  grazing.   The  present  tooth  pattern  of  equids 
developed  during  the  Miocene  and  has  not  changed  greatly  since 
that  time.   After  the  new  type  of  dentition  was  developed, 
horses  could  eat  most  any  vegetable  matter  including  harsh 
prairie  grasses.   Three  main  changes  occurred:   tooth  patterns 
changed  to  permit  grinding,  crown  height  increased  to  give 
longer  life  to  the  teeth,  and  a  cementum  layer  developed.   The 
cement  filled  the  valleys  and  pits  in  the  teeth  and  prevented 
food  from  lodging  and  decaying;  it  also  prevented  the  brittle 
enamel  crests  of  the  teeth  from  breaking.   The  tooth  system  of 
the  present-day  horse  is  highly  specialized  for  eating  grass 
which,  due  to  its  high  silica  content,  is  a  very  harsh  food. 
As  a  result,  the  teeth  of  horses  wear  down  rapidly.   As  they 
wear,  the  teeth  in  the  upper  jaw  move  down  and  those  in  the 
lower  jaw  move  up  so  that  a  grinding  surface  is  maintained  at 
the  same  level.   When  the  teeth  are  worn  to  the  roots  and  can 
no  longer  grind,  the  horse  will  starve.   Generally,  however, 
most  wild  horses  die  of  other  causes  before  this  occurs  (Simpson, 
1951). 

As  the  teeth  of  horses  changed,  undoubtedly  digestive  adapta- 
tions also  occurred,  but  since  fossil  records  leave  no  evidence 
this  cannot  be  substantiated  (Simpson,  1951). 

Although  the  horse  is  a  grazer  he  can  eat  and  apparently  survive 
on  a  wide  variety  of  foods.   In  various  parts  of  the  world 
horses  are  fed  grapevines,  leaves  of  lime,  lawn  clippings, 
garden  refuse,  garbage,  bamboo  leaves,  and  even  dried  fish 
(Ensminger,  1951). 

The  digestive  tract  of  the  horse  is  smaller  than  that  of  a 
ruminant  and  is  not  able  to  handle  as  much  roughage.   However, 
since  bacterial  action  similar  to  that  in  a  ruminant  takes  place 
in  the  caecum  and  colon,  the  horse  does  not  need  high  quality 
protein  in  its  diet.   This  is  not  true  of  young  colts,  however, 
since  much  less  bacterial  synthesis  takes  place  (Ensminger,  1951). 

The  esophageal  and  intestinal  opening  of  the  horse  are  close 
together,  thus  water  passes  quickly  through  the  stomach  and 
small  intestine.   No  food  leaves  the  horse's  stomach  until  it 
is  about  two  thirds  full.   As  the  horse  eats,  partially  digested 
food  passes  out  into  the  small  intestine  in  a  continuous  stream-- 
as  a  result,  up  to  three  times  the  capacity  of  the  stomach  may 
pass  out  during  a  large  meal.   Emptying  of  the  stomach  slows 
only  when  eating  stops,  and  the  stomach  is  never  empty  unless 
the  horse  has  not  eaten  for  several  days.   Some  of  the  digested 
food  is  absorbed  by  the  stomach  but  most  absorption  takes  place 


21 


in  the  intestines.   Within  the  small  intestine  the  food 
remains  quite  fluid  and  passes  through  rapidly. 

The  large  intestine  is  divided  into  five  components:   the 
caecum,  large  colon,  small  colon,  rectum,  and  anus.   Some 
bacterial  action  and  synthesis  of  vitamins  takes  place  in  the 
caecum,  but  the  greater  part  of  digestion,  including  bacterial 
action  and  absorption  of  nutrients,  occurs  within  the  large 
colon  (Hanauer,  1973). 

The  efficiency  of  the  extraction  of  protein  from  foods  of 
various  composition  is  similar  for  cattle  and  horses  (Glover 
and  Duthie,  1958).   There  are  important  differences,  however, 
in  the  mechanism  of  digestion  between  horses  and  ruminants. 
The  ruminant  maximizes  the  use  of  protein  at  the  expense  of 
energy.   The  mechanical  and  chemical  breakdown  of  plant  cells 
within  the  rumen  is  so  thorough  that  relatively  complete 
extraction  of  the  cell  contents  takes  place.   This  process  of 
recycling  for  efficiency  limits  the  passage  of  food  through 
the  digestive  tract.   When  the  ingested  food  contains  large 
quantities  of  lignified  cell  walls  the  rate  of  passage  is 
slowed  and  the  overall  assimilation  of  protein  may  be  quite 
low.   Under  these  conditions  the  animal  may  not  be  able  to  meet 
its  maintenance  requirements  unless  it  has  the  opportunity  to 
selectively  graze  components  of  the  vegetation  that  contain 
cells  with  thin  walls  and  a  high  concentration  of  protein. 

In  the  horse,  fermentation  of  cellulose  occurs  in  the  colon 
and  large  intestine,  but  the  simple  stomach  remains  the  prin- 
cipal site  of  protein  extraction.   The  slow  process  of  cell 
breakdown  that  occurs  in  the  ruminant  is  avoided  and  proteins 
are  quickly  assimilated  as  amino  acids.   The  horse  achieves 
almost  equal  efficiency  with  the  ruminant  in  protein  extraction 
through  quantity  rather  than  quality.   Food  passes  through  its 
digestive  system  almost  twice  as  fast  as  it  does  in  a  cow; 
therefore,  the  horse  can  support  itself  on  forage  too  low  in 
protein  to  support  a  cow  or  other  large  ruminant,  but  it  must 
maintain  a  much  higher  rate  of  intake  (Bell,  1970,  1971). 

In  general  the  smaller  the  animal  the  higher  is  its  metabolic 
rate.   However,  there  is  a  degree  of  overlap  between  ruminants 
and  non- ruminants  from  the  effect  of  body  size  on  metabolic 
rate.   For  relative  maintenance  (per  unit  weight  per  unit  time) 
small  animals  need  more  protein  and  energy  than  large  animals. 
They  will  lose  weight  more  rapidly  and  be  less  competitive  than 
a  larger  animal  if  they  are  both  on  a  submaintenance  diet.   The 
reverse  is  true  for  an  absolute  maintenance  (per  animal  per 
unit  time)  diet.  With  equal  quality  of  food  intake  the  smaller 
animal  needs  less  food  and  as  a  result  has  more  time  to  eat 
enough  for  maintenance.   The  smaller  animal  can  thus  afford  to 
be  more  selective  in  its  grazing  habits  and  survive  on  a 


22 


sparse  food  supply  that  would  starve  the  larger  animal  (Bell, 
1970,  1971). 

The  year-round  food  habits  of  present-day  wild  horses  have 
never  been  extensively  studied.   In  some  areas  where  these 
animals  still  survive,  their  habitat  includes  rough  terrain, 
sparse  vegetation  and  adverse  climate.   It  is  possible  that 
the  food  habits  of  these  animals  are  considerably  different 
from  the  animals  that  live  in  less  harsh  environments. 

Hansen  (1975,  personal  communication)  reports  that  he  has  been 
unable  to  find  a  single  scientific  publication  on  the  diet  of 
the  domestic  or  wild  horse  on  pasture  or  rangeland.   His 
studies  indicate  that,  under  ordinary  range  conditions,  80  to 
95%  of  the  diet  (on  a  dry-weight  basis)  of  wild  horses  consists 
of  grasses  and  grasslike  plants  and  that  they  consume  more 
browse  than  they  do  forbs.   Appendix  6  lists  some  of  the  plants 
Hansen  has  found  to  be  common  in  wild  horse  diets  in  the  western 
states. 

Captured  wild  horses  of  both  Asia  and  America  do  not  readily 
change  their  dietary  habits  to  include  concentrated  feeds  as 
do  their  domestic  counterparts  (Hafez  et  al,  1962). 

Dobie  (1952)  wrote  that  captured  wild  horses  preferred  cotton- 
wood  bark  to  grain  and  that  they  were  very  adept  at  pawing 
through  snow  for  their  food.   Hall  (1972)  made  random  feeding 
site  observations  during  1968  and  1969  on  the  Pryor  Mountain 
herd.   The  major  food  item  during  spring,  summer  and  fall  con- 
sisted of  grasses.   During  the  winter  they  utilized  brushy 
species,  primarily  saltbush  (Atriplex  spp.),  rabbitbush 
(Chrysothamnus  spp.)  and  big  sagebrush  (Artemisia  tridentata) , 
along  with  any  remaining  grass. 

In  Britain,  Tyler  (1972)  conducted  behavioral  studies  on  the 
semi-wild  herds  of  New  Forest  ponies.   The  herds  are  annually 
harvested  for  the  sale  of  certain  animals,  primarily  yearlings 
and  foals,  and  the  numbers  of  stallions  are  controlled.   Other 
than  this  they  lead  a  wild  existence,  remain  free  and  forage 
for  themselves  throughout  the  year. 

Their  habitat  consisted  of  deciduous  woodland,  heathland,  grass- 
land, valley  bogs,  and  open  water  in  ponds,  ditches  and 
streams. 

The  bulk  of  the  ponies1  food  during  the  summer  was  purple  moor 
grass,  Molinia  caerulea,  along  with  small  amounts  of  Agrostis, 
Festuca,  Lolium  and  Cynosurus  spp.  Certain  animals  spent  much 
time  wading  in  ponds  grazing  Glyceria  spp.,  Carex  spp.  and  most 
small  flowering  plants.  Bracken  (Pteridium  aquilinum)  was  the 
only  other  food  plant  eaten  in  quantity.   Between  autumn  and 


23 


spring,  leaves  and  shoots  of  brambles  (Rubus  spp.),  oak  (Quercus 
robar)  and  beech  (Fagus  sylvatica)  were  eaten. 

The  normal  daily  pattern  of  the  ponies  was  completely  disrupted 
when  acorns  were  abundant  as  they  spent  most  of  the  day  foraging 
for  these.   In  1964  the  deaths  of  42  ponies  were  attributed  to 
poisoning  from  eating  acorns.   When  snow  was  on  the  ground 
leaves  of  holly  (Ilex  aquifolium)  and  gorse  (Ulex  europaeus) 
provided  almost  the  only  food.   These  plants  were  grazed  as 
high  as  the  animals  could  reach  (approximately  8  feet). 

The  author  reported  that  when  hay  was  fed  during  the  winter 
there  were  a  few  groups  of  ponies  that  never  approached  the 
hay  piles  and  apparently  did  not  recognize  it  as  food. 

Feist  (1971)  spoke  only  qualitatively  on  feeding  habits  of  the 
Pryor  Mountain  herd.   Grass  was  preferred  but  in  short  supply 
so  that  the  animals  were  forced  to  supplement  their  diet  with 
other  types  of  vegetation.  Where  water  was  abundant  they  ate 
marsh  grasses,  weeds  and  forbs.   Among  woody  plants,  he  observed 
horses  grazing  the  new  growth  of  saltbush,  greasewood  (Sarcobatus 
vermiculatus) ,  black  sage  brush  (Artemisia  nova)  and  on  rare 
occasions  Utah  juniper  (Juniperus  osteosperma)  and  mountain 
mahogany  (Cercocarpus  spp.).   He  noted  horses  pawing  up  and 
eating  the  roots  of  winter  fat  (Eurotia  lanata)  and  two  species 
of  milkvetch  (Astragalus  kentrophyta  and  A.  gilviflorus) .   He 
also  reported  sighting  a  mature,  an  immature  and  a  yearling 
female  eating  old  feces  from  a  stud  pile  during  the  summer 
months. 

Appaloosa  horses  grazed  for  1%  to  3  hours  before  resting,  and 
on  the  summer  range  they  spent  much  less  time  grazing  during  the 
day  than  they  did  on  the  winter  range.   They  were  observed 
grazing  practically  all  species  of  grass  plus  elk  thistle 
(Cirsium  spp.)  and  lichens  and  bark  from  quaking  aspen  (Populus 
tremuloides) .   Although  sagebrush,  shrubby  cinquefoil  (Potentilla 
fruticosa  L.)  and  prickly  pear  cactus  (Opuntia  polycantha  Haw.) 
were  abundant  they  were  never  observed  eating  these  species 
(Blakeslee,  1974). 

When  short  of  grass,  domestic  horses  will  eat  leaves  of  trees 
and  shrubs  and  peel  bark  from  young  trees  (Smythe,  1966). 

Colostrum,  the  milk  secreted  by  the  mare  the  first  few  days 
after  partruition,  is  very  important  to  the  newborn  foal.   It 
is  not  only  different  in  chemical  composition  from  the  mare's 
normal  milk  but  contains  antibodies  to  protect  against  certain 
infections,  and  it  serves  as  a  natural  purgative  (Ensminger, 
1951). 


24 


Senses 

Vision.   The  horse  is  color  blind  and  sees  the  landscape 
ahead  as  a  mosaic  of  various  shades  of  gray  caused  by  differently 
lighted  areas.   Its  eyes  are  incapable  of  breaking  up  a  visual 
image  into  individual  items  such  as  trees,  grass,  fences,  etc. 
The  horse  recognizes  movement  through  changes  in  brightness, 
tone,  and  the  relationship  of  the  moving  subject  to  fixed 
objects.   Any  object  that  remains  perfectly  still  may  go 
unrecognized. 

The  eyes  of  the  horse  are  set  on  the  side  of  the  head  so  that 
each  eye  receives  a  different  scene.   When  the  images  are 
superimposed  the  horse  views  a  flat  panorama.   The  eye  lens  is 
non-elastic  and  the  retina  is  arranged  on  a  slope  with  the 
bottom  of  the  retina  nearer  the  lens  than  the  top.   Neither  the 
cornea  nor  the  lens  of  the  horse's  eye  is  truly  shaped,  thus 
horses  suffer  from  astigmatism;  they  have  trouble  focusing  on 
a  subject  a  little  way  ahead  and  therefore  often  mistake  a 
harmless  object  as  something  dangerous.   To  focus  on  objects 
at  different  distances  the  horse  raises  or  lowers  its  head  so 
that  the  correct  part  of  the  retina  is  on  the  subject.   Although 
the  horse  possesses  eye  muscles  to  turn  the  eye  in  various 
directions,  they  are  seldom  used  primarily  because  of  the  posi- 
tions of  the  eye  and  the  fact  that  the  neck  is  long  and  moveable. 
Generous  movement  of  the  head  is  absolutely  necessary  if  the 
horse  is  to  focus  its  eyes  properly.   When  grazing,  the  horse 
is  able  to  view  objects  from  every  direction  by  moving  its  head. 
Objects  to  the  rear  can  be  seen  between  its  legs.   A  horse  sees 
most  and  farthest  when  it  stands  still  with  head  erect  and  the 
forehead  and  muzzle  perpendicular  to  the  ground.   In  this 
position  it  can  probably  see  all  the  landscape  for  several 
hundred  yards  around  by  alternate  use  of  frontal  and  lateral 
vision.   Horses  move  the  head  into  position  to  look  straight 
ahead  and  the  ears  point  forward.   When  a  horse  is  paying 
attention  to  objects  on  either  side  of  the  body  it  cannot  see 
very  well  in  front  of  the  body;  in  like  manner  when  it  is 
staring  straight  ahead  it  cannot  see  what  goes  on  on  either  side 
(Smythe,  1966). 

Smell  and  taste.   It  is  often  difficult  to  disassociate 
taste  from  smell  in  horses.   Some  horses  will  accept  a  strong 
smell  and  refuse  on  taste,  whereas  others  will  reject  strong 
smelling  substances  without  tasting.   Domestic  horses  have  a 
liking  for  salt,  sugar,  and  honey.   They  dislike  most  strongly 
aromatic  substances  such  as  peppermint,  eucalyptus,  thyme,  and 
fats  or  anything  of  a  meaty  nature. 

The  true  nostrils  of  the  horse  open  into  the  nasal  chamber  which 
contains  large  turbinated  bones  that  are  very  brittle  and  covered 
with  mucous  membranes.   The  nasal  surfaces  contain  areas  of 


25 


closely  packed  smell  buds  and  olfactory  nerve  endings.   They 
also  have  a  false  nostril  which  runs  upward  as  a  blind  pouch. 
Horses  are  nose  breathers  with  the  nostril  size  varying  con- 
siderably among  individuals.   They  do  not  open  their  mouths  or 
drop  the  tongue  until  completely  exhausted  (Smythe,  1966). 

The  muzzle  of  the  horse  is  soft  skinned,  flexible  and  capable 
of  receiving  environmental  stimuli.   Sensory  cells  and  nerve 
endings  embedded  in  the  connective  tissues  of  the  lips  and 
nostrils  plus  the  long  bristling  hairs  that  stick  out  in  all 
directions  are  each  capable  of  receiving  sensory  stimuli.   The 
region  of  the  lips  and  nose  perform  functions  of  the  hand  in 
man.   The  nose  of  the  zebra  is  far  more  useful  for  orientation 
and  warning  against  surprise  attack  than  is  the  eye  (Burkhardt 
et  al,  1967). 

The  soft  palate  of  the  horse  is  large  and  somewhat  pendulous. 
It  is  used  to  block  the  open  end  of  the  larynx  to  prevent 
choking  when  the  horse  is  eating  or  drinking.   It  may  also  drop 
into  the  pharynx  and  limit  mouth  breathing  when  the  horse  is 
exhausted  or  nearly  so. 

Stallions  have  an  acute  sense  of  smell  and  can  smell  a  mare  in 
estrous  at  a  considerable  distance  if  she  is  upwind  (Smythe, 
1966). 

The  horse's  ability  to  smell  distant  water  has  saved  the  lives 
of  many  early  riders.   Old  time  mustangers,  when  following  a 
band  of  wild  horses,  were  very  careful  not  to  change  their 
clothing  because  their  odor  would  change.   Most  horses  can 
smell  snakes,  and  there  are  reports  of  a  horse  that  could  track 
deer.   South  American  horses  gallop  about  in  the  dark  and  avoid 
the  burrows  of  hundreds  of  Vizcacha  (Lagidium  spp.)  holes, 
probably  by  smelling  them  (Dobie,  1952). 

Domestic  horses  will  eat  a  great  variety  of  drugs  one  would 
expect  them  to  refuse.   They  are  apparently  unable  to  discrimi- 
nate between  edible  and  poisonous  plants  and,  being  unable  to 
vomit,  they  are  unable  to  get  rid  of  the  poisonous  ones  (Hafez 
et  al,  1969). 

Hearing.   Horses  hear  over  a  great  range  of  frequencies  and 
can  pick  up  sounds  too  slight  for  human  ears.   They  have  the 
advantage  of  a  long  neck  and  concave  ears  that  can  be  moved  in 
any  direction  to  not  only  detect  sound  but  also  to  pinpoint  the 
origin.   There  appears  to  be  a  connection  between  the  ears  and 
nose  of  the  horse  because  when  the  ears  prick  the  nostrils 
dilate.  When  a  horse's  ears  are  laid  back  on  the  neck  it  is 
either  a  sign  of  temper  or  an  indication  of  stress  such  as 
running  hard. 


26 


The  pitch  ranges  of  hearing  in  most  domestic  animals  is  known; 
however,  horses  are  an  exception.   There  has  been  little 
experimental  work  done  on  their  hearing. 

Horses  communicate  with  each  other  and  other  animals  with  a 
number  of  vocalizations  and  sounds.   Mares  communicate  to  their 
foals  with  a  wide  variety  of  low- toned  whinnies,  each  of  which 
appear  to  convey  a  different  meaning  (Smythe,  1966).   Dobie 
(1952)  states  that  the  scream  of  a  stallion  is  one  of  the  most 
terrifying  sounds  in  nature  and  constitutes  a  warning  to  all 
other  animals  and  stallions  within  hearing  distance. 

Domestic  horses  appreciate  sounds  from  humans,  and  there  are 
many  stories  of  grooms  who  became  famous  by  their  abilities  to 
calm  frenzied  horses  by  talking,  whispering  or  hissing  to  them. 
Highly  strung  horses  are  often  excited  or  alarmed  by  sounds 
that  are  unfamiliar  such  as  escaping  steam,  fireworks,  thunder, 
or  even  the  rustling  of  paper. 

Horses  can  sense  very  slight  ground  vibrations  through  their 
legs  and  feet.   When  grazing,  these  vibrations  are  felt  through 
the  limbs,  teeth,  jaws  and  bones  of  the  head.   The  footsteps 
of  a  man  can  be  picked  up  long  before  they  become  audible 
(Smythe,  1966).   South  American  gaucho  horses  which  lived  in  a 
semi-wild  state  expressed  great  fear  of  Indian  attacks.   They 
would  often  come  running  home  when  an  imminent  Indian  attack 
was  still  a  day's  ride  away.   A  few  frontiersmen  trained  their 
horses  to  fear  the  odor  of  Indians  (Dobie,  1952). 

Tactile  sensations.   In  order  to  maintain  its  normal  body 
temperature  of  about  100.3°  F.,  the  horse  sweats  and  shivers. 
It  has  the  ability  to  sweat  at  a  moment's  notice  from  exertion, 
fear,  or  excitement.   The  muscular  exertions  expanded  when 
working  or  running  hard  in  such  a  large  animal  makes  this  a 
necessity.   Sweat  glands  in  the  horse  occur  in  the  neck,  certain 
parts  of  the  back,  the  shoulders,  axillae,  and  groin;  however, 
there  are  no  sweat  glands  in  the  skin  of  the  limbs  except 
between  the  hind  thighs.   Foam  forms  on  the  body  and  limbs 
after  continued  exertion  when  sweat  pours  from  the  suboriferous 
skin  ducts,  and  there  is  a  considerable  amount  of  heat  loss 
through  water  vapor  from  the  breath. 

The  ability  of  the  horse  to  fairly  rattle  his  skin  to  dislodge 
insects  or  when  rising  from  the  ground  to  remove  adhering 
particles  from  the  skin  is  well  known.   This  is  made  possible 
by  the  panniculus  muscle  which  covers  the  greater  part  of  the 
body  beneath  the  skin.   The  same  muscle  is  involved  in  shiver- 
ing, which  the  horse  uses  to  increase  surface  temperatures  when 
it  is  cold. 


27 


Many  horses  respond  very  rapidly  and  often  vigorously  to  skin 
stimuli;  the  neck  and  shoulders,  the  skin  of  the  lower  limbs 
especially  around  the  feet  and  the  coronets,  the  heel  at  the 
rear  of  the  pastern,  and  the  skin  on  the  ribs  and  flanks  are 
very  sensitive.   Most  horses  appreciate  scratching  on  the  skin 
of  the  withers  and  gentle  patting  of  the  neck  and  face  (Smythe, 
1966). 

Horses  have  certain  skin  areas  that  are  extremely  sensitive  to 
certain  irritants,  either  vapor,  solids  or  liquids.   These 
include  the  lips  and  skin  of  the  muzzle,  the  skin  inside  the 
elbows,  groin,  beneath  the  tail,  the  eyelids  and  their  con- 
junctive areas,  plus  the  skin  surrounding  the  nasal  openings 
(Hafez  et  al,  1969). 


Social  Organization  and  Behavior 

General.   Behavioral  and  social  studies  of  truly  wild  horses 
are  nonexistent,  and  it  is  only  within  the  last  decade  or  so 
that  very  limited  attempts  to  study  the  wild  horses  of  the 
American  West  has  been  attempted. 

Some  form  of  close  aggregation  of  groups  of  individuals  or 
"clumping"  is  common  among  herbivores.   Animals  aggregate  for 
protection  from  predators,  mating,  and  facility  in  feeding. 
However,  according  to  Allee's  principle,  each  species  has  an 
optimum  aggregation  value.   Densities  above  or  below  this  value 
tend  to  act  as  factors  in  population  regulation  (Boughey,  1968). 

The  social  organization  of  wild  horses  is  different  than  that  of 
most  ungulates.   All  wild  horses  belong  to  a  harem  or  family 
group  controlled  by  a  dominant  male,  with  the  exception  of 
adolescent  males  that  have  been  ejected  from  the  harem  by  the 
lead  stallion,  which  form  into  small  bachelor  groups  or  bands. 
Any  wild  horse  seen  by  itself  is  either  an  adolescent  male,  a 
male  that  has  reached  maturity  and  is  trying  to  capture  its 
own  harem,  or  a  crippled,  diseased  animal  unable  to  keep  up 
with  the  group.   Hall  (1975)  states  that  a  lone  animal  normally 
is  an  old  stud  that  has  lost  his  harem.   He  never  observed  one 
of  these  older  animals  in  a  bachelor  group. 

Each  harem  also  contains  a  top-ranking  mare  that  governs  daily 
activity.   She  leads  the  group  to  forage  and  water  and  is  second 
to  the  stallion  in  command.   Whenever  the  group  runs  to  escape 
danger  this  mare  takes  the  lead  with  the  stallion  bringing  up 
the  rear  (Dobie,  1952;  Ryden,  1970).   Hall  (1972)  reported 
during  the  1971  roundup  of  the  Pryor  Mountain  horse  herd  that 
when  a  band  was  harassed,  the  lead  stallion  was  the  first  to  go 
off  by  himself.   This  was  observed  repeatedly.   He  also  commented 
that  the  instinct  to  follow  in  wild  horses  was  very  strong.   When 


28 


a  band  lost  its  lead  mare  the  remaining  horses  were  often 
completely  disoriented. 

The  stallion's  functions  are  to  breed,  keep  the  group  intact, 
prevent  social  strife,  defend  against  other  males  of  the  same 
species,  and  defend  against  any  danger  that  threatens  the 
family  group  (Dobie,  1952;  Ryden,  1970).   Hall  (1975)  doubts 
that  the  stallion  will  defend  against  any  danger  threatening 
the  group  other  than  that  posed  by  a  competing  stallion. 

The  protection  and  defense  of  a  family  group  or  harem  by  any 
animal  has  three  main  functions.   It  distributes  the  animals 
over  the  available  environment,  provides  for  the  selection  of 
the  strongest  male  by  fighting  (which  affects  the  progeny),  and 
offers  defense  for  the  young  (Lorenz,  1963). 

Stallions  control  their  groups  by  biting,  kicking,  and  by  a 
threatening  posture  that  includes  elongating  and  arching  the 
neck  and  weaving  the  head  back  and  forth.   This  threatening 
posture  is  generally  all  that  is  necessary  to  make  any  member 
of  the  group  obey  (Ryden,  1970). 

Klingel  (1972)  states  that  a  number  of  behavioral  patterns  are 
almost  identical  in  the  Equidae.   In  their  social  organization, 
however,  there  are  considerable  differences;  two  basic  patterns 
exist.   The  plains  zebra  (E.  quagga) ,  the  mountain  zebra  (E. 
zebra) ,  the  horse  (E.  przewalski)  and  possibly  the  asiatic  wild 
ass  (E.  hemionus)  live  in  harem  groups  composed  of  one  to  several 
mares  led  by  a  dominant  stallion.   Surplus  stallions  form  groups 
of  their  own.   The  young  leave  the  family  group  in  a  set  pattern, 
and  do  not  establish  territories.   These  social  units  move 
freely  over  their  home  ranges  which  they  share  peacefully  with 
conspecifics.   In  opposition  to  this  type  of  social  behavior, 
Grevy's  zebra  (E_.  grevyi)  and  the  wild  ass  (E.  af ricanus)  dis- 
play no  personal  attachments  between  any  individuals  other  than 
mare-foal  relationships.   The  animals  often  occur  singly,  in 
stallion  groups,  mare  groups,  mare-foal  groups  and  mixed  herds. 
The  groups  are  variable  and  their  composition  may  change  at  any 
time.   Some  of  the  stallions  are  territorial  but  do  not  prevent 
conspecifics  from  entering  their  territory  as  long  as  they  do 
not  interfere  with  the  mating  activities  of  the  territorial 
stallion.   They  only  defend  territories  when  there  is  an  estrous 
mare  near  the  boundary.   Only  the  territorial  male  will  court  a 
mare  at  any  one  time  because  all  the  other  males  are  subordinate 
to  him.   There  is  no  order  of  dominance  or  leadership  among 
adults.   All  adult  individuals  seem  to  be  of  equally  low  rank 
and  only  the  mare  and  her  foal  ever  search  for  each  other  when 
they  are  separated. 

Aggressiveness.   Individual  aggressiveness  in  vertebrates 
towards  members  of  the  same  species  is  expressed  in  two  ways: 


29 


defense  of  a  given  territory  and  the  establishment  of  hier- 
archies of  precedence  within  social  groups  (Collias,  1970). 

Due  to  the  potential  danger,  most  mammals  avoid  fighting  if 
possible.   They  generally  resort  first  to  aggressive  display 
and  threats  which  often  result  in  submission  or  appeasement 
by  the  weaker  opponent.   Fighting  seldom  results  in  the  death 
of  the  loser  unless  the  environment  is  overcrowded.   An  unsound 
environment  distorts  the  normal  patterns  of  social  behavior 
(Scott,  1958).   There  are  many  examples  of  increased  aggression 
in  crowded  populations  (Mathews,  1964).   Crowding  may  also 
produce  a  reverse  reaction  when  it  exceeds  certain  levels,  and 
animals  may  become  passive  and  nonreactive  (Southwick,  1970). 

When  wild  horses  reached  their  peak  numbers  during  the  last 
century  they  often  existed  in  crowded  environments  (Dobie,  1952), 
In  the  Pryor  Mountains,  Hall  (1972)  reports  that  they  still  do, 
which  may  account  for  the  sometimes  conflicting  reports  on 
behavioral  patterns  at  different  times  and  places  among  wild 
horse  bands. 

Many  stallions  are  very  domineering  and  keep  close  watch  over 
members  of  their  group.   Only  during  foaling  will  the  stud 
permit  a  mare  to  leave  the  band  so  that  she  can  find  a  secluded 
spot  to  foal  (Dobie,  1952;  Ryden,  1970).   Hall  (1972)  reported 
that  some  mares  appeared  to  be  fickle  and  were  observed  away 
from  their  group  for  several  days  at  a  time.   He  also  observed 
extreme  variation  in  the  amount  of  time  mares  remained  away  from 
the  group  during  foaling  (one  day  to  several  weeks).   He  attrib- 
uted this  variation  to  the  strength  of  her  bonds  with  the  group. 

If  a  mare  with  foal  falls  behind  during  an  escape  attempt  the 
stud  may  kill  the  foal  by  breaking  its  neck  or  he  may  force  the 
mare  to  abandon  it.   Studs  also  may  often  display  threatening 
gestures  toward  humans  while  the  band  is  escaping.   If  another 
stallion  approaches  the  harem,  the  two  stallions  posture  with 
tails  arched  and  heads  pressed  together  staring  at  each  other 
until  one  backs  down.   If  a  fight  ensues,  they  rear  on  their 
hind  legs,  ears  back  and  mouth  agape.   They  strike  with  their 
forefeet,  bite  for  the  jugular  and  often  wheel  and  kick.   During 
the  encounter  they  shriek,  snort  and  scream.   If  one  falls  he 
is  stomped  by  the  other.   The  winning  stud  seldom  chases  his 
rival  for  more  than  a  short  distance  (Dobie,  1952;  Hall,  1972; 
Ryden,  1970). 

The  intolerance  for  other  males  is  greater  during  the  breeding 
season  than  it  is  at  other  times  and  reaches  its  lowest  ebb 
during  the  winter  months  (Hall,  1972). 

All  age  classes  of  appaloosas  threaten  foals,  but  adult  mares 
are  more  aggressive  toward  foals  not  their  own  than  are  other 


30 


age  groups  (Blakeslee,  1974).   All  pony  mares  were  aggressive 
to  strange  foals  and  either  threatened,  kicked  or  bit  them  if 
they  came  close  (Tyler,  1972).   Feist  (1971)  did  not  find  any 
consistent  pattern  of  dominant  hierarchy  among  the  individual 
mares  of  the  various  bands  in  the  Pryor  Mountain  horses.   Ex- 
hibition of  authority  by  one  mare  over  another  appeared  to 
depend  on  a  particular  situation  and  the  superior  dominance  of 
the  stud  overshadowed  any  formation  of  hierarchy  among  the 
mares.   Hall  (1972),  who  spent  a  great  deal  more  time  with 
these  horses  than  Feist,  reported  a  definite  recognizable 
hierarchy  within  each  band  and  that,  once  established,  it 
remained  fairly  constant.   Blakeslee  (1974)  stated  that,  among 
adult  females,  neither  age,  size,  weight,  nor  length  of  time  in 
the  group  determined  dominance.   The  only  observed  character- 
istic common  to  dominant  mares  was  their  tendency  to  be  aggres- 
sive.  Tyler  (1972)  noted  kicking  fights  between  pony  mares  on 
several  occasions,  presumably  to  establish  or  maintain  dominance. 
The  rank  order  of  mares,  once  established,  was  very  stable  and 
a  mare  dominant  over  another  mare  in  one  situation  was  never 
subordinate  to  the  same  mare  in  another  situation.   It  was 
also  noted  that  a  subordinate  mare  rarely  challenged  a  previously 
established  more  dominant  mare.   The  length  of  time  a  mare  lived 
in  a  group  was  not  important  in  determining  rank,  and  a  foal 
shared  status  with  its  mother  only  when  it  was  close  to  her. 
The  rank  order  of  mares  in  any  one  group  or  where  several  groups 
were  considered  was  a  simple  linear  one,  but  with  large  numbers 
the  order  was  complicated  by  triangular  relationships  within 
the  hierarchy.   In  one  group  of  eight  dominant  mares  there  was 
a  significant  correlation  between  their  size  and  rank.   However, 
this  did  not  hold  true  with  other  groups  and  neither  size  nor 
age  explained  the  high  position  of  some  mares  in  hierarchies. 

Dominance  between  stallions  and  mares  varied.   Stallions  always 
dominated  where  food  was  concerned  but  not  in  all  other  situa- 
tions . 

Family  groups.   The  number  of  mares,  yearlings  and  colts 
controlled  by  a  stallion  varies.   Dobie  (1952)  reports  viewing 
as  many  as  a  hundred  horses  in  a  group  but  adds  that  15  to  20 
was  the  usual  size.   Other  reports  state  that  normal  groups 
consist  of  one  to  eight  mares  exclusive  of  colts  and  yearlings 
(Hoyt,  1886).   Wild  horse  bands  in  the  Pryor  Mountains  and  in 
Nevada  averaged  three  to  four  animals  composed  of  a  stallion, 
lead  mare,  subordinate  mares,  yearlings,  or  colts  (Hall,  1972; 
Pellegrini,  1971).   When  wild  horses  are  frightened  or  stampede, 
the  bands  may  join  together  into  one  large  herd  controlled  by  a 
single  stallion.   However,  the  large  bands  again  separate  into 
individual  harems  when  they  stop  running  (Dobie,  1952;  Ryden, 
1970). 


31 


Stallion  colts  are  usually  ejected  from  the  band  at  an  early 
age,  about  two  to  four  years.   The  age  at  which  they  are 
expelled  seems  to  depend  on  the  aggressiveness  of  the  dominant 
stallion.   These  young  males  may  remain  alone  near  where  they 
were  born  until  they  can  join  up  with  a  bachelor  group.   If 
they  join  together  they  are  controlled  by  a  dominant  stallion 
in  a  loosely  organized  group.   Early  writers  reported  as  many 
as  25  of  these  young  males  in  a  band.   Later  reports  indicate 
two  to  five  animals  varying  from  two  to  five  years  of  age. 
When  stallions  approach  the  age  and  size  at  which  they  can 
capture  a  harem  of  their  own  they  may  leave  the  group  and 
remain  alone  (Dobie,  1952;  Hall,  1972;  Ryden,  1970). 

Hall  (1972,  1975)  stated  that  there  was  often  a  considerable 
interchange  of  animals  between  harems.   Many  such  interchanges 
were  observed  in  the  Pryor  Mountain  study.   He  relates  the 
interchanges  and  the  small  band  size  to  the  sex  ratio  (607o  males, 
407o  females)  ,  and  to  overcrowding.   He  reported  that  in  some 
areas  in  Utah  the  band  size  averages  six  animals.   In  these 
cases  the  sex  ratio  is  about  equal  and  there  is  no  evidence 
of  overcrowding. 

Klingel  (1965)  reported  that  family  groups  among  the  plains 
zebra  (Equus  quagga)  are  extremely  stable  and  are  not  held 
together  by  the  dominant  stallion.   In  five  observed  cases 
where  the  lead  stallion  disappeared,  the  groups  remained 
together  until  subsequently  taken  over  by  another  stallion. 

Sometimes  large  harems  have  two  stallions;  both  may  breed,  but 
one  is  always  dominant.   When  the  herd  is  in  flight  this  second 
stallion  runs  abreast  and  forward  of  the  male  in  command  (Dobie, 
1952;  Hall,  1972;  Ryden,  1972). 

Home  range.   In  any  discussion  of  home  ranges  or  territories 
of  America's  wild  horses  it  is  well  to  remember  that  these  two 
items  may  be  those  imposed  or  strongly  influenced  by  man.   Field 
studies  that  will  enable  us  to  understand  the  diversity  of  ways 
these  animals  use  the  space  available  to  them  are  virtually 
nonexistent. 

Every  species  of  mammal  has  a  home  range  of  some  type.   These 
home  ranges  may  be  stationary  or  they  may  change  with  conditions. 
The  ranges  often  overlap  and  migratory  animals  may  have  more 
than  one  home  range.   A  home  range  may  be  defined  as  the  area 
over  which  an  animal  or  a  group  of  animals  travel  in  pursuit  of 
routine  activity.   It  may  have  no  clearly  defined  boundaries 
but  it  must  produce  the  energy  requirements  of  the  specific 
group  of  animals  that  occupy  it.   It  implies  a  self-imposed 
restriction  of  movement  (Burt,  1943). 


32 


Estes  (1974)  claims  that  the  actions  of  zebras  are  found  in  all 
members  of  the  horse  family;  therefore,  the  only  truly  wild 
member  of  the  Equidae  that  may  furnish  clues  as  to  how  wild 
horses  once  lived  is  the  plains  or  Burchell's  zebra  (Equus 
Burchelli).   There  are  still  over  a  half  million  of  these 
animals  on  the  savannah  of  northern  South  Africa  and  the  southern 
Sudan.   During  their  annual  migrations  herds  of  zebras  may 
stretch  across  the  grasslands  as  far  as  one  can  see. 

Plains  zebras  live  in  stable  families,  or  harems,  of  mares  and 
foals,  with  each  family  of  six  to  sixteen  members  controlled 
and  defended  (mostly  from  other  males)  by  one  stallion.   Large 
troops  of  zebras  are  made  up  of  these  units  plus  bachelor  herds 
which  mainly  consist  of  immature  stallions. 

Both  Feist  (1971)  and  Hall  (1972)  reported  on  home  ranges  within 
the  Pryor  Mountain  herd.   Hall  observed  that  the  ranges  varied 
seasonally  with  an  individual  band  using  as  many  as  five  areas. 
He  observed  at  least  17  bands  of  horses  using  the  same  area  at 
least  part  of  the  time.   These  actions  were  attributed  to  con- 
gestion and  lack  of  adequate  forage.   Feist  reported  that  home 
ranges  were  relatively  well  defined  and  adhered  to  and  only 
rarely  did  a  band  leave  their  home  range.   Pellegrini  (1970) 
reported  that  the  home  range  of  the  three  bands  most  extensively 
studied  in  the  Wassuk  Range  of  Nevada  confined  their  activity 
to  cube-shaped  areas,  while  the  home  range  of  lone  horses  was 
linear  along  the  boundaries  of  two  adjacent  home  ranges.   Among 
the  New  Forest  ponies,  home  range  depended  on  the  topography 
and  the  nature  of  the  habitat.   Four  requirements  for  a  home 
range  were  necessary:   grazing  area,  shelter,  water  and  shade. 
When  these  were  close  together  the  home  ranges  were  often  small 
(200  acres).   The  home  ranges  of  the  different  groups  were  very 
stable  and  though  they  were  often  almost  identical  or  overlapped, 
each  group  tended  to  use  a  different  part  of  the  grazing  area 
(Tyler,  1972). 

When  the  wild  horses  were  at  their  peak  during  the  last  century 
many  writers  reported  large  herds.   Early  maps  of  Texas  marked 
the  territory  between  the  Rio  Grande  and  Nueces  Rivers  as  "vast 
herds  of  wild  horses."   A  Franciscan  missionary  reported  that 
when  crossing  an  area  uninhabited  by  people  the  wild  horses  were 
so  abundant  that  their  trails  made  the  area  look  like  the  most 
populated  in  the  world  and  that  all  the  grass  was  gone.   Within 
these  large  herds,  however,  the  individual  bands  or  harem  groups 
retained  their  identity  (Dobie,  1952). 

Lorenz  (1963)  lists  several  species  of  animals  (bison,  antelope, 
and  horses)  which  do  not  maintain  strict  home  ranges  or  display 
territorial  jealousy  if  there  is  enough  food  for  all. 


33 


Indians  depended  on  the  home  range  instinct  to  hold  their  semi- 
wild  herds  on  reservation  lands,  but  not  all  horses  reveal  this 
trait  and  as  a  result  many  of  them  reverted  to  a  wild  state 
(Ryden,  1970). 

Mustangs  confine  their  feeding  and  flights  from  danger  to 
certain  boundary  lines.   This  fact  was  well  known  to  plainsmen 
who  made  a  business  of  catching  wild  horses  (Cook,  1919). 

Smith  (1841)  stated  that  the  genuine  wild  horse  is  migratory, 
moving  north  in  the  summer  and  returning  in  early  fall.   The 
mixed  races  (feral  horses)  move  to  pastures  rather  than  to 
points  of  the  compass. 

Territoriality.   A  territory  in  animal  populations  may  be 
defined  as  that  part  of  the  home  range  which  is  protected  from 
individuals  of  the  same  species  either  by  fighting  or  by  other 
aggressive  action.   Territoriality  is  so  widespread  among  the 
vertebrates  and  in  many  of  the  invertebrates  that  it  must  be 
considered  a  basic  animal  characteristic.   Although  it  is  not 
found  in  all  animals  nor  is  it  always  developed  within  strict 
boundaries  among  the  animals  that  possess  it,  the  potential 
is  there  whether  the  animal  displays  the  characteristic  or 
not  (Burt,  1943). 

There  is  only  scant  evidence  to  indicate  the  degree  of  terri- 
toriality displayed  by  wild  horses. 

Pellegrini  (1971),  in  his  study  of  Nevada  herds,  concluded  that 
wild  horses  establish  a  territory  even  though  no  special  section 
of  the  home  range  exists  as  a  defended  area.   Hall  (1972)  reports 
that  horses  in  the  Pryor  Mountains  do  not  establish  territories, 
and  he  is  convinced  that  they  are  not  territorial.   The  indi- 
vidual bands  maintain  a  "sphere  of  intolerance,"  an  area  near 
the  harem  or  family  group  in  which  the  dominant  male  will 
defend  against  other  males.   This  "sphere"  (the  distance  within 
which  another  stallion  may  approach  the  harem)  expands  during  the 
breeding  season  and  contracts  after  it  is  over.   During  the 
winter  months  it  may  be  almost  nonexistent. 

Feist  (1971),  reporting  on  the  same  herd  of  horses,  concluded 
that  the  bands  or  studs  do  not  exhibit  territorial  behavior. 
They  do,  however,  maintain  a  spacing  between  bands.   In  all 
observations  this  spacing  was  related  to  distance  between  bands 
and  never  to  the  defense  of  a  ground  area. 

The  dominant  males  of  the  plains  zebra  do  not  defend  a  territory. 
They  control  moveable  property  rather  than  real  estate  (Estes, 
1974). 


34 


Vocalizations.   Waring  (1971)  states  that  social  inter- 
actions are  common  among  both  feral  and  domestic  horses.   These 
interactions  occur  through  visual,  auditory,  olfactory, 
gustatory  and  tactile  cues.   He  warns  against  using  any  single 
means  of  communication  out  of  context.   To  fully  understand 
the  importance  of  sounds,  all  other  means  of  communication 
must  be  identified  and  understood. 

Using  American  Saddle  Bred  horses  as  subjects,  he  tape  recorded 
their  vocalizations,  then  by  spectrographic  patterns  he  divided 
them  into  seven  basic  categories.   Identified  were  squeals, 
nickers,  whinnies,  groans,  blows,  snorts,  and  snores-- the  first 
four  vocal,  the  latter  three  non- vocal.   Breed  variability  may 
exist,  but  it  probably  falls  within  the  basic  sound  categories 
described. 

Squeals:   express  threats.   Nickers:   anticipatory  sounds  prior 
to  being  fed;  stallions  in  courtship;  and  by  mares  when  foals 
are  in  jeopardy.   Whinnies:   uttered  during  distress  or  inquiry. 
Groans:   issued  during  discomfort  or  anguish.   Blows:   express 
alarm  or  aid  olfactory  investigation.   Snorts:   used  in  conflict 
situations-- they  may  express  relief  or  disgust.   Snores:   pro- 
duced during  inhalation,  two  types  were  detected--one  of  short 
duration  uttered  prior  to  emitting  an  alarm  blow,  the  second, 
of  longer  duration,  produced  while  the  animal  was  prone.   The 
author  indicates  that  all  of  these  sounds  are  used  by  either 
sex. 

Feist  (1971)  identified  four  definite  vocalizations  in  the 
Pryor  Mountain  herd:   the  snort,  neigh,  nicker,  and  scream.   He 
observed  the  snort  as  a  danger  signal  used  mostly  by  stallions, 
seldom  by  mares.   The  neigh  was  used  as  a  distress  call  primarily 
by  mares  and  younger  horses  of  either  sex.   Studs  used  the 
neigh  to  call  animals  that  had  strayed  or  when  they  were  in  a 
build-up  to  a  fight.   The  nicker  was  used  in  closed  communica- 
tion and  courtship.   The  scream  was  emitted  exclusively  by 
studs  during  fights. 

Hafez  et  al  (1962)  identifies  the  snort  as  a  danger  signal,  the 
neigh  or  whinny  as  a  distress  call,  and  the  nicker  as  a  sign  of 
relief  or  for  closed  communication  and  courtship. 

Tyler  (1972)  reported  that  the  squeal  was  most  commonly  uttered 
by  mares  when  a  stallion  approached  and  sniffed  them.   Mares 
also  squealed  when  fighting  or  when  displaying  aggressiveness. 
Stallions  only  occasionally  squealed  when  fighting. 

Postures  and  facial  expressions.   Tyler  (1972)  states  that 
six  expressions  are  recognized  in  equids  but  they  are  not  all 
seen  in  true  horses.   She  describes  five  of  these  expressions 
among  the  New  Forest  semi-wild  ponies. 


35 


Ponies  yawned  by  holding  their  mouths  wide  open  with  the  upper 
and  lower  teeth  exposed  and  ears  forward.   Yawning  occurred 
before  or  after  resting,  when  mares  in  estrous  were  being 
sniffed  by  stallions,  in  stallions  after  copulating,  and  in 
foals  after  suckling. 

The  greeting  expression  occurred  when  two  group  companions  or 
a  stallion  and  a  mare  met;  they  sometimes  extended  their  heads 
and  touched  each  other's  muzzle  and  lips.   These  often  developed 
into  threat  postures  when  the  ponies  laid  back  their  ears.   The 
greeting  expression  was  also  used  as  a  preliminary  to  mutual 
grooming. 

During  the  flehmen  posture  the  animal  extends  its  neck  and  curls 
the  upper  lip  so  that  the  teeth  are  exposed.   Schneider  (1930, 
1931,  1932a,  b)  described  the  flehmen  in  a  wide  variety  of 
ungulates.   Hafez  et  al  (1969)  states  that  this  expression 
commonly  takes  place  during  the  precopulatory  patterns  of  many 
ungulates.   Estes  (1974)  states  that  during  the  flehmen,  urinary 
odors  are  apparently  assayed  in  an  accessory  olfactory  system 
specialized  to  detect  concentrations  of  sex  hormones.   In  the 
New  Forest  ponies  (Tyler,  1972)  the  expression  was  always 
observed  as  a  reaction  to  some  smell:   by  stallions  after 
sniffing  a  mare;  by  stallions,  mares  or  foals  after  sniffing 
urine;  by  mares  or  other  ponies  after  sniffing  fresh  placentae 
or  by  any  pony  at  the  sight  of  another  showing  the  flehmen 
posture.   Blakeslee  (1974)  stated  that  the  flehmen  posture  was 
exhibited  by  foals,  yearlings,  and  adults  in  a  variety  of 
situations  other  than  smelling  urine  on  the  ground.   The 
posture  was  induced  by  touching  the  nose  of  a  horse  with  a 
human  hand  and  by  sight  of  another  horse  urinating,  or  by  the 
sight  of  another  horse  exhibiting  the  flehmen  posture.   Feist 
(1971)  observed  55  situations  of  this  action  in  the  Pryor 
Mountain  herd.   All  of  them  were  related  to  olfactory  responses 
and  all  of  them  were  by  males. 

The  threat  expression  is  characterized  by  backward-directed 
ears  and  was  first  described  by  Antonius  (1937).   Tyler  states 
that  it  is  absent  in  asses,  half-asses,  and  Grevy's  zebra 
(Equus  grevyi) ♦   The  true  horses  do  not  draw  up  the  corners  of 
the  mouth  as  do  the  other  species  of  zebra.   In  the  New  Forest 
ponies  mild  threats  were  exhibited  by  slightly  laying  back  the 
ears,  but  intense  threats  were  expressed  with  the  ears  flat 
back  and  sometimes  the  mouth  slightly  open,  and  occurred  just 
before  a  dominant  pony  attempted  to  bite  a  subordinate  pony. 
Stallions  also  used  the  threat  gesture  when  driving  mares  by 
stretching  the  neck  toward  the  ground,  ears  flat,  and  swaying 
the  head  back  and  forth.   Ryden  (1970)  observed  similar  actions 
when  stallions  threatened  or  drove  mares. 


36 


Zeeb  (1959)  described  the  snapping  expression.   It  is  character- 
ized by  stretching  of  the  neck  with  the  ears  slightly  back  and 
down,  the  corners  of  the  mouth  drawn  back  partly  exposing  the 
teeth,  and  the  lower  jaw  in  motion  up  and  down.   Tyler  (1972) 
stated  that  the  expression  was  common  in  the  New  Forest  ponies 
when  foals  or  yearlings  were  threatened  or  approached  by  adult 
mares  or  stallions.   It  occurred  in  a  variety  of  occasions, 
most  of  which  seemed  to  be  released  by  fear.   Occasionally  it 
was  a  response  of  foals  and  even  yearlings  to  approaching  cows. 
Although  it  was  regarded  as  a  submissive  expression  it  had  no 
effect  in  preventing  older  animals  from  carrying  out  their 
threats.   Foals  and  yearlings  were  often  bit  or  kicked  by  mares 
even  after  they  had  made  the  snapping  response.   Feist  (1971) 
recorded  21  instances  of  this  behavior  by  young  males  to  the 
dominant  stallion.   Ordinarily  the  stallion  made  no  recognition 
of  the  submissive  behavior. 

Grooming.   Domestic  horses,  unlike  cattle,  do  not  groom 
each  other  but  they  may  nibble  each  other  along  the  withers  or 
stand  head  to  tail  and  flick  flies  off  each  other's  faces. 
Nibbling  of  the  withers  is  usually  a  sign  of  recognition  and 
often  may  be  noted  when  two  animals  meet  after  an  interval 
(Hafez   e_t  al,  1969). 

Trumler  (1958)  classified  grooming  in  zebras  as  follows:   local- 
ized muscle  contractions;  shaking;  striking  one  part  of  the 
body  against  another  or  against  the  ground;  rubbing  (includes 
rolling);  scratching;  nibbling;  and  social  or  mutual  grooming. 
Feist  (1971)  stated  that  mutual  grooming  was  a  common  part  of 
the  daily  activities  in  the  Pryor  Mountain  wild  herd,  accomplished 
by  pulling  the  lips  back  and  using  the  incisor  teeth  to  groom 
the  neck,  withers,  base  of  the  mane  and  on  down  to  the  rump. 
Horses  also  groomed  one  side,  then  switched  to  groom  the  other 
side.   Mutual  grooming  occurred  only  between  herd  members  and 
almost  all  grooming  combinations  were  observed  except  between 
the  stud  and  immature  males.   Tyler  (1972)  observed  all  the 
above  forms  of  grooming  in  the  semi-wild  New  Forest  ponies. 
Mutual  grooming  was  a  response  to  insect  infestations,  shedding 
of  hair,  or  as  a  means  of  social  contact.   Each  pony  had  only 
a  small  number  of  partners  with  which  it  groomed.   These  part- 
ners were  usually  related,  unrelated  group  companions,  or 
members  of  two  closely  associated  groups.   The  usual  grooming 
partner  of  a  foal  was  its  mother,  another  sibling  or  another 
foal.   Mutual  grooming  bouts  were  most  often  initiated  by  the 
subordinate  animal  and  ended  by  the  dominant  animal.   Mutual 
grooming  was  a  common  activity  among  all  age  classes  of  appa- 
loosas.   All  observations  of  grooming  were  within  their 
particular  group,  and  there  was  no  consistent  pattern  of 
preference  for  certain  partners  (Blakeslee,  1974). 


37 


Feist  (1971)  observed  81  cases  of  rolling,  50  of  which  were  in 
dust  and  31  in  mud  and  water.   Although  yearlings  and  foals 
rolled,  no  female  yearlings  or  foals  were  observed  rolling  in 
dust  or  mud.   Dusting  sites  were  scattered  throughout  the  range 
and  were  used  by  all  bands  or  horses  that  passed  by.   Ponies 
usually  rubbed  against  fixed  objects  such  as  stumps,  trees  or 
wooden  bridges.   When  insects  were  abundant  they  spent  long 
periods  stamping,  kicking  at  their  bellies,  shaking  and  lashing 
their  tails  or  rolling.   Rolling  often  occurred  at  the  end  of 
a  resting  period  or  when  they  encountered  wet  grass  after  a 
rain,  patches  of  bare  ground  or  sawdust.   No  special  sites  were 
reserved  for  rolling  (Tyler,  1972).   Appaloosa  horses  choose 
specific  pieces  of  ground  for  rolling  places,  and  these  were 
so  well  used  that  they  were  denuded  of  vegetation  and  covered 
with  dust.   Rolling  in  water  was  common  for  all  age  classes  as 
was  rubbing  against  trees  or  other  fixed  objects  (Blakeslee, 
1974). 

Play.   Hie  word  play  is  a  human  concept  applied  to  any 
activity  other  than  work.   The  term  is  difficult  to  apply  to 
wild  animal  activity  or  behavior;  since  animals  do  not  work 
(in  the  human  sense  of  the  word)  they  cannot  be  said  to  play. 

Human  play  no  longer  has  survival  value;  however,  in  animals 
it  must  be  assumed  that  it  does  have  value.   Play  occurs 
among  animals  only  when  they  are  free  of  environmental  or 
physiological  stresses.   It  is  a  widely  held  view  that  play 
is  preparation  for  adult  activity,  and  the  animals  that  have  an 
opportunity  to  play  prior  to  becoming  adults  have  a  selective 
advantage  over  those  animals  that  are  denied  this  opportunity. 
However,  it  has  never  been  clearly  shown  that  animals  prevented 
from  playing  are  less  efficient  as  adults  than  those  that  play 
(Loizos,  1966).   Play  increases  locomotive  skills  and  may  be 
used  to  test  social  dominance  (Hafez  e_t  al,  1969). 

Feist  (1971)  reported  that  play  among  foals  of  the  same  band 
and  foals  of  other  bands  occurred  regularly.   This  was  tolerated 
by  the  studs  if  the  juveniles  returned  to  their  own  bands  after 
playing.   Play  involved  running,  kicking,  sudden  stops  and 
starts,  and  head  tossing.   Foals  were  observed  playing  at  all 
times  of  the  day  but  primarily  in  early  evening  when  tempera- 
tures had  cooled.   Play  among  other  age  groups  was  rare  and  if 
started  was  quickly  ended  by  the  dominant  male.   Tyler  (1972) 
reported  that  play  in  young  foals  took  the  form  of  the  foal 
galloping  to  and  from  its  mother  or  making  irregular  circles 
around  her.   It  bucked  and  kicked  as  it  galloped.   Sometimes 
the  foal  reared  up  at  its  mother  or  pulled  at  her  mane  and 
neck.   Young  foals  were  very  inquisitive  and  often  sniffed  or 
nibbled  at  strange  objects  and  then  rushed  back  to  their 
mother.   Play  between  foals  did  not  occur  before  their  second 


38 


or  third  week.   After  this  period,  play  between  foals  became 
more  common.   Foals  nibbled,  groomed,  or  chased  one  another 
and  kicked  out  with  their  hind  legs  and  bucked  or  reared  as 
they  galloped.   The  play  of  colts  older  than  four  weeks  differed 
from  that  of  fillies.   Colts  spent  long  periods  play  fighting, 
rearing,  pawing  and  attempting  to  bite.   Most  play  fights 
between  colts  contained  all  the  components  of  true  fighting 
between  adults.   Play  between  fillies  which  often  ended  as 
mutual  grooming  was  much  less  common  than  play  between  colts 
or  between  colts  and  fillies.   Both  colt  and  filly  foals  showed 
interest  in  stallions  and  often  attempted  to  approach  them. 
Most  stallions  were  very  tolerant  and  usually  ignored  the  foals. 
Play  fights  were  observed  between  stallions  and  colts  but  never 
between  stallions  and  colts  over  two  years  old.   Blakeslee 
(1974)  observed  similar  play  among  foals  but  did  not  report  any 
differences  between  the  play  of  males  and  females. 

Sleep  and  rest.   In  lying  down  the  horse  brings  all  of  its 
legs  under  the  body,  bends  its  knees  and  hocks  and  permits  the 
chest  to  touch  the  ground  before  the  hind  quarters.   When  down, 
the  horse  either  rests  on  one  side  of  its  chest  with  a  fore  and 
hind  leg  underneath  or  it  lies  on  its  side  with  all  legs 
stretched  out.   In  getting  up,  the  forelegs  are  stretched  out 
first  followed  by  the  rear  legs  pushing  up  the  hindquarters. 
The  horse  may  sleep  very  soundly  for  seven  hours  out  of  twenty- 
four,  mostly  during  the  heat  of  the  day.   Horses  sleep  standing 
up  or  laying  down;  however,  it  is  rare  to  see  all  horses  in  a 
group  laying  down  at  the  same  time;  one  is  always  alert  (Hafez 
et  al,  1969).   Many  horses  sleep  on  their  feet  and  some  graze 
all  night  (Ensminger,  1951).   Ruckebusch  et  al  (1970)  studied 
behavior  and  physiological  responses  of  horses  during  sleep. 
They  state  that  sleeping  while  standing  was  rare  in  experimental 
animals  but  appeared  to  be  frequent  in  animals  maladjusted  or 
insecure  in  their  surroundings.   Dobie  (1952)  wrote  that  wild 
horses  never  lie  down  altogether,  many  sleep  on  their  feet  and 
lay  down  only  to  roll.   He  relates  the  story  of  an  old-time 
horseman  who  told  him  that  wild  horses  napped  regularly  three 
times  during  the  night,  at  about  nine,  eleven  and  just  before 
dawn.   Feist  (1971)  observed  a  number  of  situations  in  which 
horses  rested  or  were  sleeping.   Foals  spent  a  great  deal  of 
time  sleeping  while  lying  down;  this  decreased  as  they  grew 
older.   Mature  animals  only  occasionally  lay  down  and  no  obser- 
vations were  made  of  an  entire  band  lying  down.   Yawning  and 
stretching  often  occurred  following  sleep  or  rest.   Pellegrini 
(1971)  stated  that  horses  in  the  Wassuk  range  of  Nevada  travelled 
little  during  the  night.   They  either  stayed  near  water  or  in 
sheltered  areas,  apparently  sleeping  most  of  the  time.   They 
also  rested  or  slept  in  shaded  areas  during  the  day.   Both  foals 
and  adults  spend  more  time  resting  during  the  winter  than  in 
the  summer  (Blakeslee,  1974;  Tyler,  1972).   The  latter  author 
attributed  this  difference  to  increased  food  availability  during 
the  summer. 


39 


Pawing.   The  original  function  of  pawing  was  part  of  the 
horse's  nutritive  behavior  and  always  occurred  together  with 
an  olfactory  investigation  (Odberg,  1972). 

Horses  paw  through  snow  to  obtain  grass  or  they  may  paw  to 
remove  undesirable  objects  (stones,  cactus)  that  interfere  with 
or  block  their  efforts  to  reach  desirable  forage  plants.   Paw- 
ing was  also  used  to  search  for  water  from  dried  waterways 
where  it  was  common  for  early  cowboys  to  find  holes  dug  by 
horses  as  much  as  eight  feet  deep.   Pawing  is  also  their  means 
of  breaking  ice  to  obtain  water  (Boone,  1933;  Dobie,  1952; 
Odberg,  1972;  Smythe,  1966). 

Horses  paw  for  various  other  reasons.   They  have  often  been 
observed  sniffing  and  pawing  the  areas  where  they  are  about 
to  roll.   Pawing  may  occur  during  the  threatening  behavior  when 
two  males  meet  and  they  may  also  paw  during  courtship  if  thwarted 
in  their  attempts  to  mate.   Pawing  and  sniffing  often  accompany 
investigation  of  stud  piles.   Foals  sometimes  paw  when  they  are 
frustrated  in  their  attempts  to  feed,  and  mares  may  paw  before 
foaling.   Domestic  horses  paw  at  the  sight  of  unreachable  food 
given  by  humans  or  while  waiting  for  an  action  they  feel  or 
"know"  is  going  to  happen  (before  a  race) ,  or  they  may  paw  from 
boredom  or  nervousness  when  they  are  in  their  stable  (Boone, 
1933;  Dobie,  1952;  Odberg,  1972). 

Eliminative  behavior.   Vigorous  adult  horses  defecate  5  to 
12  times,  and  urinate  7  to  11  times  per  day  (Hafez  e_t  al ,  1969). 
Tyler  (1972)  estimated  frequency  of  elimination  in  the  New 
Forest  ponies.   Adult  mares  defecated  on  an  average  of  once  in 
just  over  2  hours  and  urinated  once  every  4  to  4%  hours.   Foals 
urinated  about  once  per  hour  in  the  first  week  or  so,  but  this 
gradually  dropped  to  the  same  frequency  as  adults  when  they 
were  about  a  year  old.   Stallions  frequently  sniffed  piles  of 
feces  and  then  defecated  or  less  commonly  urinated  on  the  pile. 
Sometimes  they  did  this  several  times  in  succession  on  different 
piles  of  feces. 

Horses,  unlike  cattle,  stop  whatever  they  are  doing  to  urinate 
or  defecate.   They  maintain  a  characteristic  stance  according 
to  sex  when  eliminating;  and  after  elimination  the  stallion, 
but  not  the  mare,  smells  the  eliminative  product  (Hafez  et  al, 
1969).   Ponies  stopped  grazing  to  urinate  but  merely  raised 
their  tails  and  continued  to  graze  while  defecating  (Tyler, 
1972). 

Feist  (1971)  believes  that  there  is  much  more  significance  in 
the  eliminative  behavior  of  wild  horses  than  there  is  in 
domestic  horses.   Particularly  important  is  the  action  of  the 
stud  in  relation  to  other  horses  in  the  band  and  to  other 


40 


studs.   The  stud's  urination  behavior  is  directed  to  excrements 
of  the  mare,  whereas  his  defecation  behavior  is  used  primarily 
for  the  stud  pile.   Feist  further  believes  that  this  behavior 
is  dominance- linked. 

Blakeslee  (1974)  found  no  evidence  of  dominance  display  by 
appaloosa  stallions  connected  with  eliminative  behavior  nor  did 
they  accumulate  stud  piles  like  wild  horses. 

Scent  or  visual  boundary  marking.   The  fact  that  wild  horses 
deposit  their  fecal  matter  in  one  place  and  thus  create  piles 
of  manure  commonly  referred  to  as  "stud  piles"  is  well  known. 
There  is  little  evidence  as  to  the  exact  purpose  of  this  type 
of  behavior.   It  may  be  a  form  of  scent  marking,  visual  boundary 
marking  or  both. 

Dobie  (1952)  gives  an  account  of  the  many  early  explorers, 
historians,  and  others  who  were  amazed  at  the  numerous  piles  of 
horse  dung  that  lined  the  trails  used  by  wild  horses  on  both 
the  South  American  pampas  and  on  the  western  prairie.   He  states 
that  a  stallion's  dung  piles  constitute  a  visual  and  olfactory 
notice  to  other  stallions  that  he  had  been  there. 

Shenkel  (1947)  concluded  that  scent  marking  legitimizes  the 
leader,  marks  his  territory  and  aids  in  making  new  acquaintances. 
Lyall-Watson  (1964)  interprets  scent  marking  differently,  stating 
that  scent  marking  aids  the  animal  in  maintaining  familiarity 
with  his  environment  and  assures  him  that  he  is  within  his 
home  range.   This  interpretation  suggests  that  scent  marking  is 
not  used  as  an  aggressive  display  for  territorial  defense  even 
though  it  would  be  effective  in  maintaining  a  territory. 
Pellegrini  (1971)  indicated  that  mares  and  colts  also  accumu- 
late dung  piles  and  that  these  piles  probably  served  as 
boundary  markers.   Hall  (1972)  discounts  the  purpose  of  stud 
piles  for  marking  territory  because  the  Pryor  Mountain  horses 
are  not  territorial  and  the  piles  appeared  to  be  randomly 
scattered.   Feist  (1971)  agrees  with  Hall.   "If  five  bands 
passed  by  a  particular  pile  on  a  trail  or  on  a  feeding  area 
in  one  day  it  would  be  rare  if  all  of  the  studs  did  not  defecate 
with  accompanying  posturing  on  that  pile." 

Zebra  stallions  mark  only  the  feces  of  mares  in  estrous 
(Klingel,  1969,  1972).   He  suggested  that  scent  marking  in 
zebras  is  vestigial  behavior  and  probably  inherited  from  terri- 
torial ancestors.   The  same  author  later  reported  that  the 
marking  behavior  of  the  non- territorial  equids  has  no  known 
function.   Kleiman  (1966)  distinguished  scent  marking  from 
true  elimination  by  three  factors,  one  of  which  was  directional 
posture.   Pony  stallions,  when  marking,  oriented  their  elimina- 
tive products  toward  other  piles  of  feces.   They  often  walked 
from  one  pile  of  feces  to  another  stopping  to  eliminate  on 


41 


several  piles.   Stallions  usually  defecated  when  marking;  mares 
and  foals  more  commonly  urinated  (Tyler,  1972).   Both  Feist 
(1971)  and  Tyler  (1972)  reported  that  the  amount  eliminated  by 
stallions  when  marking  was  controlled  and  expelled  in  small 
quantities,  compared  with  true  elimination.   Feist  observed 
stallions  stepping  over  a  mare's  excretion  and  urinating  in  a 
short  but  strong  blast.   Stallions  were  also  observed  defecating 
up  to  three  times  during  a  short  period  while  fighting.   Tyler 
noted  that  there  was  no  evidence  that  marking  had  any  effect  on 
other  stallions. 

Water  and  watering  behavior.   There  is  no  data  available 
on  the  amount  of  water  needed  or  consumed  by  wild  horses  under 
their  various  habitat  conditions.  (Ensminger  (1951)  states  that 
the  average  domestic  horse  will  consume  about  12  gallons  per 
day.   Stoddart  et  al  (1975)  quotes  from  early  research  on 
domestic  horses  and  lists  10-12  gallons  per  day. 

Among  wild  horses  Feist  (1971)  noted  no  consistent  watering 
schedule  by  any  band.   Generally  each  band  visited  a  water  hole 
once  a  day  except  during  very  hot  weather  when  they  watered 
twice  a  day.   Over  most  of  the  range,  horses  were  always  within 
four  or  five  miles  of  a  water  hole.   The  time  spent  at  water 
holes  rarely  exceeded  30  minutes.   Pellegrini  (1971)  observed 
territorial  behavior  of  wild  horses  in  Nevada  at  water  holes. 
When  two  bands  arrived  simultaneously  one  band  was  dominant. 
The  less  dominant  band  did  not  water  until  the  other  band  had 
left  the  area.   Hall  (1972)  states  that  in  the  cooler  months 
horses  trail  a  considerable  distance  to  water  but  during  the 
warm  periods  of  late  June,  July,  and  August  they  concentrate 
on  water.   Frei  (1975)  states  that  wild  horses  in  eastern 
Nevada  will  trail  as  far  as  10  to  15  miles  to  water  and  seldom 
stay  any  closer  to  water  than  three  miles.   When  temperatures 
reach  their  maximum  (95°  F.)  during  the  summer  the  horses  water 
every  other  day.   Actual  drinking  time  generally  does  not  exceed 
3-4  minutes  per  horse,  and  they  rarely  remain  near  the  water 
hole  more  than  5  minutes  after  drinking.   Appaloosa  horses 
rarely  remained  long  at  watering  places,  usually  no  longer  than 
a  half  hour.   Although  the  horses  used  well  worn  trails  going 
to  and  from  water,  they  had  no  set  pattern  for  time  of  arrival. 
They  trailed  to  water  at  a  certain  time  for  several  days  in 
succession  and  then  changed  their  schedule  (Blakeslee,  1974). 

Charles  Goodnight,  a  scout  during  the  Civil  War,  told  Dobie 
(1952)  that  wild  horses,  unless  severely  disturbed,  did  not 
graze  more  than  five  miles  from  water  and  that  they  watered  at 
the  same  spot  each  day.  During  droughts  wild  horses  of  the 
South  American  pampas  sometimes  became  so  frantic  that  upon 
finding  water  they  piled  into  it  on  top  of  each  other,  drowning 
and  trampling  to  death  large  numbers  (Dobie,  1952).   Smith  (1841) 


42 


reports  similar  behavior  in  South  American  wild  horses  during 
drought  periods.   The  thirsty  animals  trampled  each  other  so 
furiously  that  thousands  of  skeletons  lined  their  watering 
places.   He  suggested  that  these  actions  were  probably  a  means 
of  population  control. 

Foal  behavior.  The  following  has  been  extracted  from  a 
paper  by  Waring  (1970)  who  studied  foal  behavior  in  American 
Saddlebreds. 

Within  seconds  of  birth  the  foal  raises  its  head  and  assumes  a 
sternal  position.   By  pawing  and  by  motions  of  the  head  and 
neck  the  foal  attempts  to  move  away  from  the  mare;  this  drags 
the  hind  legs  free  of  the  mare's  vagina  and  eventually  severs 
the  umbilical  cord. 

Within  15  minutes  the  foal  attempts  to  rise  but  often  not  until 
three-quarters  of  an  hour  has  passed  do  the  hind  legs  flex 
sufficiently  to  permit  it  to  stand.   The  eyes,  open  at  birth, 
show  distinct  binocular  orientation  by  25  minutes.   The  mare 
vocalizes  to  her  foal  within  minutes  after  birth,  and  auditory 
orientation  of  the  foal  occurs  within  45  minutes.   The  initial 
stance  is  unsteady  and  the  foal  shifts  frequently  to  maintain 
its  balance.   Suckling  motions  have  been  observed  within  55 
minutes.   Nursing  depends  partly  on  the  mare's  willingness  to 
stand  still  or  position  herself  so  the  foal  can  find  her 
nipples.   Some  foals  nurse  soon  after  standing  and  some  do  not 
succeed  until  almost  2  hours  old.   Defecation  may  occur  prior 
to  one  hour  of  age  and  urination  a  few  hours  later. 

At  one  hour  of  age  the  foal  commonly  shows  ability  in  standing 
and  moving  about,  can  see  and  hear,  can  eliminate,  express 
curiosity  and  care-seeking  behavior.   During  the  second  hour  the 
colt  perfects  its  walking  and  shows  evidence  of  its  attachment 
to  its  mother;  the  mare  and  colt  may  vocally  respond  to  each 
other's  sounds.   Sleep  begins  as  short  naps  and  increases  until 
it  occupies  much  of  the  foal's  early  life.   Fear  of  new  objects 
begins.   When  it  is  two  hours  old,  the  foal  may  walk  easily, 
lie  down,  nurse,  follow  its  mother,  vocalize,  seek  shelter 
beside  her,  express  fear,  and  sleep. 

By  half  a  day  it  can  walk,  trot,  gallop,  combat  insects  (by 
use  of  its  tail  or  by  kicking  and  nipping) ,  urinate  in  a 
manner  characteristic  of  its  sex,  exhibit  short  periods  of 
animated  play,  and  consume  small  amounts  of  forage.   The  mare- 
to-foal  bond  begins  soon  after  birth  and  is  stronger  than  the 
reciprocal.   When  the  two  are  separated,  the  mare  may  show 
extreme  excitement,  but  the  foal  exhibits  disorientation  more 
than  distress. 


43 


Tyler  (1972)  observed  foal  behavior  of  the  semi-wild  ponies. 
Almost  immediately  after  birth  an  observed  foal  had  pushed  its 
muzzle  and  a  foreleg  through  the  amniotic  membranes.   Within 
five  minutes  it  attempted  to  stand,  but  this  was  thwarted  by 
the  mare's  vigorous  licking  action.   It  stood  within  33  minutes 
and  suckled  successfully  52  minutes  after  birth.   Most  mares 
began  to  lick  their  foals  a  few  minutes  after  birth  and  con- 
tinued to  do  so  for  as  long  as  a  half  hour.   On  the  foals'  first 
day  the  mares  are  very  possessive  and  keep  the  foals  away  from 
all  other  ponies.   On  the  second  day  the  foals  are  usually  able 
to  recognize  their  mothers  by  smell,  and  the  mares  become  less 
protective.   A  few  foals  ate  or  nibbled  at  grass  on  their  first 
day,  and  some  were  observed  in  such  activities  as  rubbing, 
rolling,  scratching,  stamping,  shaking  and  nibbling.   They  also 
yawned  and  displayed  the  flehmen  posture  and  snapping  expression, 
First-week  foals  seldom  strayed  more  than  25  yards  from  their 
mothers  and  even  those  up  to  5  months  old  spent  less  than  107o 
of  their  time  more  than  50  yards  away  from  her.   When  a  foal 
approached  its  mother  to  suckle,  it  often  nickered,  laid  back 
its  ears  and  tossed  its  head.   The  foal  then  moved  in  front  of 
the  mother  pushing  under  her  head  before  it  attempted  to  suckle. 
It  did  this  regardless  of  the  direction  of  approach.   The  close 
relationship  between  the  mare  and  her  yearling  continued  even 
though  the  mare  had  a  new  colt.   The  yearling  rested  near  its 
mother,  groomed  with  her  and  followed  her.   Weaning  usually 
occurred  when  foals  were  about  a  year  old  or  at  the  time  of 
the  birth  of  a  new  foal.   The  author  observed  one  pony  mare 
which  allowed  her  yearling  to  nurse  after  the  birth  of  her 
foal,  but  considered  this  behavior  atypical.  Blakeslee  (1974) 
stated  yearlings  were  not  generally  allowed  to  nurse  after  the 
birth  of  the  new  foal.   However,  if  a  mare  did  not  foal,  her 
yearling  frequently  continued  to  nurse. 

Tyler  (1972)  reported  that  until  their  third  or  fourth  month, 
pony  foals,  due  to  their  short  necks,  straddled  their  forelegs 
to  graze.   The  average  amount  of  time  spent. grazing  by  foals 
increased  from  3.5  minutes  per  hour  the  first  week  to  16.3 
minutes  per  hour  after  the  fourth  month.   Foals  in  their 
twelfth  and  thirteenth  months  spent  a  mean  of  44.4  minutes  per 
hour  grazing. 

The  author  also  reported  that  after  birth,  licking  of  the  fetal 
fluids  by  the  mare  from  herself  and  the  foal  seemed  to  be  very 
important.   After  a  mare  has  licked  her  foal,  she  is  able  to 
discriminate  between  it  and  other  foals,  and  a  relationship 
between  mother  and  young  becomes  established.   The  great  attrac- 
tion to  the  fetal  fluids  was  shown  by  a  pony  mare  whose  colt 
fell  into  a  ditch  soon  after  birth.   The  foal  was  rescued  and 
moved  a  few  yards  away  from  the  mother,  but  the  mare  remained 
near  the  ditch  and  the  placenta.   When  the  colt  was  carried  back 
to  her  she  began  to  lick  it  and  a  normal  relationship  was 
established. 


44 


Blakeslee  (1974)  stated  that  foals  pull  at  and  mouth  various 
plants  when  one  or  more  days  old;  however,  they  do  not  swallow 
the  plant  material  until  their  teeth  have  cut  through  the  gums 
at  about  the  fifth  day.   Grooming  in  foals  did  not  occur  until 
they  were  about  a  month  old,  and  the  initial  bouts  usually 
were  with  their  mothers.   Very  young  foals  had  trouble  drinking 
from  streams,  and  it  took  them  several  days  to  learn  to  proper- 
ly spread  their  forelegs  and  retain  their  balance  while  drinking. 

Coprophagy  in  foals.   Tyler  (1972)  reported  that  coprophagy 
was  common  in  pony  foals  up  to  three  or  four  weeks  old.   The 
feces  eaten  almost  always  belonged  to  the  foal's  mother,  but 
on  two  occasions  a  foal  was  observed  eating  its  own  feces. 
Blakeslee  (1974)  also  reported  young  foals  eating  feces, 
usually  their  mothers'.   Older  foals  did  not  eat  feces.   Hafez 
et  al  (1962)  report  that  adult  domestic  horses  reject  the 
feces  of  their  own  kind  but  that  foals  eat  a  considerable  amount 
of  fresh  feces  of  adult  horses.   They  attribute  this  behavior 
to  the  need  for  proper  bacterial  flora  for  the  foal's  intestines, 

Predation  and  Disease 

Predation.   As  far  as  is  known,  there  are  no  scientific 
studies  of  predation  on  wild  horses.   The  mountain  lion  (Felis 
concolor)  is  the  only  large  predator  remaining  within  their 
habitat  that  is  capable  of  killing  an  adult  horse.   It  is 
possible  that  coyotes  could  kill  foals  if  they  were  unprotected. 
Feral  dogs,  when  hunting  in  packs,  certainly  have  the  capability 
to  attack  and  kill  horses,  but  whether  they  do  or  do  not  has 
never  been  reported. 

The  only  natural  enemies  of  horses  are  mountain  lions  and  wolves, 
All  horses  are  afraid  of  bears  but  even  in  California  where 
grizzlies  were  once  numerous,  they  killed  few  horses.   Wolves 
sometimes  attacked  horses  but  they  preferred  buffalo  and  after 
these  were  gone  they  turned  to  cattle.   The  natural  prey  of 
the  mountain  lion  is  deer,  but  once  they  had  tasted  horsemeat 
(especially  foal)  it  became  one  of  their  favorite  foods  (Dobie, 
1952). 

Young  and  Goldman  (1946)  cite  instances  of  mountain  lions  prey- 
ing on  horses  in  Arizona,  Colorado  and  New  Mexico.   Raising 
colts  and  even  maintaining  a  herd  of  adult  horses  was  impossible 
in  some  areas  due  to  lion  predation.   Once  lions  came  addicted 
to  horse  flesh  they  often  abandoned  all  caution.   The  writers 
also  note  that  mountain  lions  are  exceptionally  fond  of  burro 
meat. 

Diseases,  pests  and  parasites.   Outbreaks  of  diseases  or 
infestations  by  pests  and  parasites  within  a  wild  animal  popu- 
lation are  often  important  indicators  of  other  problems.   These 


45 


may  be  related  to  habitat,  poor  nutrition,  overcrowding, 
competition,  injury,  harassment,  or  other  reasons. 

The  more  common  afflictions  of  domestic  horses,  unless  other- 
wise indicated,  have  been  adapted  fromEnsminger  (1951).   It 
is  possible  that  any  one  or  more  of  the  diseases  could  be 
present  in  or  transmitted  to  wild  horse  herds.   Symptoms, 
treatment  and  details  are  beyond  the  scope  of  this  technical 
note. 

Equine  abortion:   Causes  may  be  grouped  into  four  categories 
those  resulting  from  infection  by  Salmonella  abortivoequina ; 
those  from  streptococci  infection  which  gains  entrance  through 
the  genital  tract;  a  virus  or  epizootic  type  which  is  highly 
contagious;  and  those  abortions  that  occur  from  miscellaneous 
causes  which  cannot  be  classified  into  the  above  categories. 
These  may  vary  from  accidents  or  injuries  to  nutritional  or 
endocrine  disturbances. 

Equine  encephalomyelitis  (sleeping  sickness):   a  disease 
carried  by  4  immunological,  distinct  filtrable  viruses.   It  is 
vectored  by  13  members  of  3  genera  of  mosquitos  of  which  Culex 
tarsalis  is  the  most  likely  carrier.   It  may  also  be  carried 
by  spotted  fever  ticks  (Dermacentor  venustus)  and  assassin  bugs 
(triatoma  spp . ) . 

Equine  infectious  anemia  (swamp  fever):   a  very  serious 
blood  disease  of  horses  and  mules.   It  is  caused  by  a  specific 
virus  that  may  stay  in  the  host  for  years.   Treatment  has  been 
unsuccessful. 

Infectious  adenitis:  Also  referred  to  as  strangles  or 
distemper,  the  disease  is  caused  by  a  bacterial  streptococcii. 
Transmission  is  usually  by  the  inhalation  or  ingestion  of  the 
infected  discharges.   It  is  highly  contagious  and  the  organisms 
may  live  outside  the  animal's  body  for  as  long  as  six  months. 

Glanders  or  farcy:  A  very  old  disease  of  bacterial  origin. 
It  may  be  diagnosed  through  the  "mallein  test."  No  cure  is 
known. 

Dourine:   A  chronic  venereal  disease  of  horses  and  asses 
commonly  termed  equine  syphilis.   It  is  caused  by  a  protozoa 
and  is  spread  mostly  through  mating,  but  may  be  transmitted 
by  biting  insects. 

In  1930  the  Bureau  of  Animal  Industry  reported  that  177o  of  the 
wild  horses  on  the  San  Carlos  Apache  Indian  lands  were  infected 
with  dourine.   Later  tests  indicated  80%  of  the  horses  in  the 
high  country  were  infected.   The  Federal  Bureau  of  Animal 
Husbandry  removed  about  500  horses  from  Nevada  wild  herds  in 


46 


1935  because  of  the  presence  of  the  disease  (Wyman,  1945).   The 
Bureau  of  Land  Management  in  1974  reported  a  suspected  out- 
break of  the  same  disease  in  wild  horses  near  China  Lake, 
California. 

Rabies:   caused  by  a  filtrable  virus  which  usually  occurs 
from  injected  saliva  in  a  bite  wound;  an  acute  infectious 
disease  of  horses  and  all  other  warm-blooded  animals,  including 
man. 

Anthrax:   also  infectious  to  all  warm-blooded  animals.   The 
bacillus  of  anthrax  can  survive  in  the  soil  for  very  long  periods, 
It  was  the  first  disease  in  which  immunization  was  accomplished 
by  an  attenuated  culture  by  Pasteur  in  1881. 

Tetanus:   Caused  by  an  extremely  powerful  toxin  liberated 
by  the  tetanus  organisms  (Clostridium  tetanii) .   The  organism 
is  found  in  certain  soils,  horse  dung,  and  sometimes  in  human 
excreta. 

Periodic  opthalmia,  or  moon  blindness:   the  most  common 
cause  of  blindness  in  horses  and  mules.   It  is  an  inflammation 
of  the  inner  portion  of  the  equine  eye  and  its  associated 
structures.   Horses  of  all  ages  are  susceptible,  and  it  may 
occur  in  one  or  both  eyes  (Roby  et  al ,  1956). 

Parasites:   The  species  and  degree  of  harm  vary  in  different 
parts  of  the  world.   They  may  be  located  in  practically  every 
tissue  and  cavity  of  the  body.   Some  are  specifically  localized, 
others  are  migratory  through  different  parts  of  the  body.  The 
most  important  of  these  are:   the  bot  flies,  of  which  there  are 
three  species;  the  strongyles  with  six  species,  the  larger  of 
which  are  commonly  referred  to  as  bloodworms  or  redworms;  the 
ascarids  or  roundworms;  two  species  of  pinworms;  four  species 
of  stomach  worms;  the  screw  worm,  largely  confined  to  the 
south  and  southwest;  blowflies;  ringworm;  lice;  mites;  and 
ticks. 

Poisonous  plants:   Horses  often  eat  and  may  be  poisoned  by 
many  species  of  poisonous  plants.   Some  plants  may  be  eaten 
over  extended  periods  before  producing  ill  effects,  others 
that  contain  acute  poisons  may  produce  visible  symptoms  or 
death  soon  after  being  eaten  (Huffman  e_t  _al,  1956). 

In  addition,  horses  are  subject  to  colds,  laryngitis,  bronchitis, 
pneumonia  and  pleurisy  (Hanauer,  1973). 


47 


Competition  and  Relationships  With  Other  Animals 

Competition.   The  extent,  nature  and  degrees  of  competition 
between  wild  horses  and  other  domestic  or  wild  animals  for 
habitat  components  such  as  food,  water,  space  and  cover  or  other 
requirements  has  never  been  investigated.   Cook  (1968),  writing 
on  the  nutritive  content  of  range  forage  for  domestic  ruminants, 
stated  that  the  most  critical  period  for  grazing  animals  that 
inhabit  seasonal  ranges  are  those  months  between  December  and 
April  when  inclement  weather  and  perhaps  poor  range  conditions 
cause  animals  to  lose  weight  excessively.   When  range  conditions 
are  poor,  the  degree  of  utilization  of  the  forage  increases 
and  the  digestibility  and  nutrient  content  decreases  because 
animals  are  forced  to  eat  the  less  nutritious  parts  of  the 
plants.   Thus  nutritional  deficiencies  are  common  on  winter 
ranges  of  the  intermountain  region.   The  above  would  also 
apply  to  wild  horses  in  varying  degrees  over  much  of  their 
range.   Since  wild  horses  are  on  the  range  year  round,  at  some 
season  or  seasons  they  occupy  the  same  range  as  domestic  live- 
stock, elk,  deer,  and  antelope.   If,  during  these  periods, 
forage  is  in  short  supply  the  various  classes  of  herbivores 
will  compete,  and  it  is  likely  that  the  less  dominant  animals 
will  suffer  the  most. 

Hansen  (1975)  does  not  think  that  wild  horses  compete  strongly 
with  mule  deer  (Odocoileus  hemionus)  or  antelope  on  most  ranges, 
but  he  would  expect  them  to  compete  with  cattle  since  their 
diets  appear  to  be  60  to  98  percent  similar.   They  may  compete 
moderately  with  domestic  sheep,  bighorn  sheep  (Ovis  canadensis) 
and  elk  (Cervus  canadensis) . 

Both  the  Bureau  of  Land  Management  and  the  Forest  Service 
report  competition  between  wild  horses,  domestic  livestock, 
and  big  game  animals.   Generally  these  reports  have  centered 
around  forage  supply. 

Cook  (1975)  states  that  in  many  areas  where  wild  horses  now 
occur  the  habitat  is  unsuitable  for  year-round  feed  requirements, 
Under  the  provisions  of  the  Wild  Horse  Act  they  cannot  be  moved 
to  suitable  areas  if  that  particular  area  did  not  previously 
contain  wild  horses.   The  author  estimates  that  wild  horses 
are  increasing  20  to  30%  per  year,  and  under  these  conditions 
many  of  them  will  starve  and  the  range  will  deteriorate.   Cook 
feels  that  starvation,  disease,  and  deterioration  of  the 
environment  may  not  be  acceptable  management  measures. 

Relationships  with  other  animals.   Documented  information 
on  the  relationships  between  wild  horses  and  other  animals  is 
extremely  scarce. 


48 


In  1828  a  Mexican  surveying  party  reported  that  between  Laredo 
and  San  Antonio,  Texas,  deer  and  wild  horses  grazed  together 
in  large  numbers.   When  wild  horses  were  frightened  and  ran  in 
large  bands,  they  were  sometimes  joined  by  antelope  (Antilocapra 
americana)  (Dobie,  1952).   Ryden  (1970)  stated  that  wild  horses 
and  buffalo  (Bison  americana)  often  grazed  together  and  that 
antelope  used  the  horses  for  sentries  to  warn  them  of  danger. 
During  severe  winters  cattle  often  followed  the  horse  herds  so 
they  could  graze  in  areas  the  horses  had  opened  up.   Other 
reports  conflict  with  these  statements.   Linsdale  and  Tomich 
(1953)  reported  that  mule  deer  (Odocoileus  hemionus  columbianus) 
moved  away  when  horses  grazed  too  close  to  them.   Both  Blakeslee 
(1974)  and  Tyler  (1972)  wrote  that  free-roaming  appaloosas  and 
semi-wild  ponies,  respectively,  dominated  cattle.   Tyler  ob- 
served a  yearling  pony  threaten  a  fallow  deer  doe  (Dama  dama) 
that  was  grazing  near  it. 

McKnight  (1959)  wrote  that  many  of  the  respondents  to  his 
questionnaire  stated  that  wild  horses  abused  livestock,  some- 
times killing  or  crippling  them  and  sometimes  excluding  them 
from  watering  places. 


Wild  Horse  Management 

(The  following  sections  on  Population  Management  and  Population 
Control  were  contributed  by  Milton  Frei  of  the  Bureau  of  Land 
Management,  Denver,  Colorado.) 

Population  management.   The  management  of  wild  horses 
presents  a  new  challenge  to  public  land  administration  agencies 
such  as  the  Bureau  of  Land  Management  and  the  U.  S.  Forest 
Service.   Heretofore  management  responsibilities  of  these 
agencies  have  been  limited  to  animal  habitat  rather  than  the 
animals  themselves. 

Although  the  concept  of  wild  horse  population  management  is  a 
relatively  new  one,  the  principles  of  animal  population  manage- 
ment are  well  documented  and  can  be  applied  directly  to  wild 
horses. 

The  first  step  in  managing  wild  horse  populations  is  to  deter- 
mine the  number  of  animals  to  manage  in  any  particular  area. 
The  determination  of  this  number  must  be  based  on  available 
habitat  and  consideration  of  other  animal  species  or  resource 
values. 

Once  the  number  of  wild  horses  to  be  retained  for  management 
has  been  determined,  the  next  step  is  to  analyze  those  factors 
which  have  molded  the  population  into  what  it  is  today.   It 
must  be  true  that  over  the  long  run,  as  many  wild  horses  die 


49 


as  are  born.   This  is  the  same  scheme  that  nature  has  built 
into  all  of  her  animal  species.   Therefore,  before  management 
of  wild  horse  populations  can  begin,  the  factors  of  population 
dynamics  (productivity,  mortality,  sex  ratio  and  age  structure) 
must  be  collected  and  understood.   These  factors  can  then  be 
analyzed  to  determine  the  forces  which  have  shaped  the  popula- 
tion and  to  predict  the  numerical  abundance  of  wild  horses  in 
the  future. 

The  first  step  in  a  wild  horse  population  analysis  is  to  deter- 
mine if  the  population  is  stable,  increasing  or  decreasing. 
The  following  formula  represents  one  method  for  determining 
the  stability  of  a  wild  horse  population: 

A  =  Estimated  number  of  adults  in  population  (1  year  and  older) 

B  =  Foal/100  adults  (percent) 

F  =  Number  of  foals 
Zf  =  Mortality  of  foals  (percent) 
Nf  =  Mortality  of  foals  (number) 
Za  =  Mortality  of  adults  (percent) 
Na  =  Mortality  of  adults  (number) 

Y  =  Total  population  estimate  (adults  and  foals) 

P  =  Projected  population 

I  =  Population  increase  or  decrease 

(A)  (B)  =  F 
(F)  (Zf)  =  Nf 
(A)  (Za)  =  Na 

A  +  F  =  Y 

Y  -  (Nf  +  Na)  =  P 

P  -  A  =  I  (increase  or  decrease).   If  P  is  less 

than  A,  reverse  P  and  A  in  formula.   Values 
will  then  be  decrease  in  population. 

I  =  Population  increase  where  P>  A 


I  =  Population  decrease  where  P<  A 


Once  the  stability  of  a  wild  horse  population  has  been  deter- 
mined, it  is  necessary  to  analyze  other  population  data  prior 
to  actual  manipulation  of  the  population.   For  example,  if  the 
population  is  determined  to  be  increasing  in  total  numbers  and 
it  is  desirable  to  decrease  total  numbers,  an  analysis  can  be 
made  as  to  the  ratio  of  male  animals  to  female  animals  in  the 
total  population.   It  may  be  possible  to  decrease  the  produc- 
tivity of  wild  horses  by  increasing  the  number  of  male  animals 
in  relation  to  the  number  of  female  animals. 


50 


In  another  example,  if  the  population  is  determined  to  be  stable, 
it  is  important  to  understand  the  reasons  why.   It  may  be  that 
births  are  equalling  deaths  or  that  the  population  is  on  the 
brink  of  disaster.   In  this  example,  an  analysis  can  be  made 
as  to  the  age  structure  of  the  population.   If  the  age  structure 
is  balanced  (i.e.  all  age  classes  adequately  represented), 
nothing  more  in  the  way  of  management  need  be  done.   However, 
if  one  or  more  age  classes  are  lacking  or  totally  missing,  it 
may  indicate  that  the  missing  age  classes  must  be  restored  if 
the  population  is  to  survive. 

Population  control.   Control  of  wild  horse  populations 
differs  from  control  of  big  game  populations  in  that  they  are 
not  a  huntable  or  game  species.   Shooting  of  wild  horses  by 
persons  other  than  officials  of  the  Bureau  of  Land  Management 
or  U.  S.  Forest  Service  is  prohibited  by  federal  law  and  is 
socially  unacceptable. 

Control  of  wild  horses  is  also  restricted  in  that  it  is  a 
violation  of  federal  law  to  use  aircraft  or  motorized  vehicles 
to  capture  or  kill  a  wild  horse.   As  a  result,  the  only  tech- 
niques available  to  capture  the  animals  alive  involve  time- 
consuming  techniques  such  as  water  trapping,  dry  trapping, 
roping  and  immobilizing.   This  is  complicated  by  the  fact  that 
many  areas  are  too  rough  or  have  too  many  water  sources  for 
these  techniques  to  be  effective. 

Disposing  of  wild  horses  which  have  been  captured  presents 
additional  complications  in  population  control.   If  animals  are 
destroyed  the  problem  arises  as  to  what  should  be  done  with 
their  carcasses.   It  is  against  federal  law  to  convert  the 
remains  of  wild  horses  into  commercial  use.   If  live  animals 
are  given  away  to  private  individuals  for  keeping  under  humane 
conditions,  the  problem  arises  as  to  transfer  of  title  to  the 
animal.   It  is  not  possible  to  transfer  title  to  wild  horses 
and  as  a  result,  many  individuals  are  reluctant  to  keep  a  wild 
horse  under  those  conditions.   In  addition,  wild  horses  are 
just  as  their  name  suggests,  "wild."   Full-grown  horses  are 
very  powerful  animals  and  can  be  extremely  dangerous  when 
placed  in  the  hands  of  unexperienced  individuals. 

Methods  of  capture.   Hall  (1974)  has  prepared  a  paper  on 
wild  horse  capture  techniques  based  primarily  on  experience 
gained  during  the  herd  reduction  program  on  the  Pryor  Mountain 
wild  horse  range.   Rather  than  attempt  to  repeat,  the  reader 
is  referred  to  this  publication.   The  author  states  that  many 
of  the  methods  described  will  prove  too  costly  in  terms  of 
manpower  and  money  to  capture  large  numbers  of  horses  for 
population  reductions.   However,  as  far  as  is  known,  this  is 
the  only  publication  of  its  kind  in  existence. 


51 


Other  states,  notably  Wyoming,  Nevada,  and  Oregon,  have  had 
experience  in  gathering  wild  horses.   The  Bureau  of  Land 
Management  in  Wyoming  presented  a  paper  to  the  National  Advisory 
Board  for  wild,  free-roaming  horse  and  burros  at  their  September 
1974  meeting  in  Reno,  Nevada  on  the  techniques  and  problems  of 
wild  horse  capture. 

The  methods  used  to  capture  wild  horses  after  World  War  II  and 
prior  to  the  enactment  of  PL  92-195  have  been  described  else- 
where.  Methods  of  capture  used  by  the  early  horse  catchers 
included  creasing,  roping,  snaring  and  running  the  animals  into 
some  type  of  corral  or  trap.   Shooting  a  horse  in  the  upper 
part  of  the  neck  close  to  the  spinal  column  was  termed  creasing. 
The  shock  stunned  the  animal  so  that  he  was  immobile  until  he 
could  be  tied.   The  method  was  more  legend  than  truth.   Old 
mustangers  related  that  for  every  horse  caught  by  this  method 
fifty  were  killed.   Horses  were  snared  by  attaching  a  loop  of 
rope  to  a  long  pole.   The  rider  rode  alongside  the  horse  and 
dropped  the  loop  over  his  head.   He  then  dismounted  and  choked 
the  horse  to  the  ground.   Lassoing  or  roping  did  not  become  a 
common  method  of  capture  until  late  in  the  19th  century.   Corrals 
or  traps  were  of  all  types,  depending  upon  the  available  material 
and  the  topography  such  as  narrow  canyons  or  ravines.   Brush  was 
often  piled  fan  shaped  as  wings  to  guide  the  horses  into  the 
trap  (Dobie,  1952). 

Hoyt  (1886)  describes  wild  horse  capture  techniques  on  the 
Texas  panhandle.   The  horses  were  chased  for  5  or  6  days  by 
relays  of  saddle  horses  and  riders  until  they  were  so  exhausted 
they  would  mill  rather  than  run  when  a  rider  approached.   They 
were  then  roped  and  clog  chains  attached  to  their  legs.   Part 
of  the  clog  was  a  free  length  of  chain  which  wrapped  itself 
around  the  forelegs  if  the  horse  attempted  to  run.   All  horses 
except  yearlings  and  two-year-olds  were  clogged.   The  stallions 
were  captured  first  and  castrated  at  the  time  the  clogs  were 
attached.   The  horses  were  driven  to  water  once  a  day  and 
allowed  to  graze  only  at  night  so  that  they  would  spend  the 
hours  of  darkness  eating  rather  than  attempting  to  escape. 

Ryden  (1970)  was  told  by  old  timers  that  early  mustangers  in 
the  Wyoming  Red  Desert  captured  wild  mares,  tamed  them,  then 
sewed  their  nostrils  together  with  rawhide  until  they  could 
only  partially  breathe.   The  mares  were  then  released  to  run 
with  their  old  band.   Since  they  could  not  run  fast  they  slowed 
the  whole  band  and  the  horses  were  easier  to  capture.   Another 
method  used  was  to  bend  a  horseshoe  around  a  captured  mare's 
ankle  and  then  release  her.   The  horseshoe  did  not  bother  her 
when  she  walked  but  when  she  ran  it  banged  against  her  other 
legs  and  slowed  her.   These  mares  were  used  over  and  over 
again. 


52 


Both  James  and  Catlin  writing  in  1823  and  1838,  respectively, 
relate  that  the  Comanches  could  tame  and  ride  a  wild  horse 
within  a  day  or  so  of  capture.   A  small  party  of  well-mounted 
Indians  would  hide  in  a  narrow  ravine  or  some  other  concealed 
area  while  the  other  members  of  the  tribe  would  drive  the  horses 
into  the  ambush  area.   Each  horse  was  captured  with  a  lasso  or 
noosed  rope  around  the  neck.   They  were  then  quickly  thrown  and 
their  heels  tied  together. 

Wild  horse  management  plans.   Both  the  U.  S.  Forest  Service 
and  the  Bureau  of  Land  Management  have  developed  plans  for  pro- 
tection, management  and  control  of  wild  horses.   Both  agencies 
have  land  use  planning  systems  that  evaluate  the  resource  and 
then  develop  integrated  planning  and  management  for  all  the 
multiple  uses  of  the  land  area  under  consideration.   These 
include  the  vegetative  and  watershed  conditions,  wildlife  needs, 
livestock  use,  recreational  use,  and  other  legitimate  demands. 

Current  problems.   From  various  reports  these  include: 
rapidly  expanding  populations  that,  due  to  the  restrictions  of 
PL  92-195,  will  demand  costly  and  inefficient  means  of  control; 
lack  of  biological  data  on  wild  horses;  lack  of  valid  censusing 
and  population  data;  lack  of  funds  and  manpower  needed  for 
intense  management;  competition  between  livestock,  elk,  deer, 
and  antelope  for  forage  and  habitat;  problems  of  disposing  of 
excess  horses;  management  and  control  of  horses  on  intermingled 
lands  (federal,  private,  state  or  other  ownership);  stallions 
stealing  privately  owned  mares;  highway  hazards  (Wyoming  Bureau 
of  Land  Management  reports  that  at  least  10  horses  were  killed 
by  cars  in  1974);  and  recreationists  have  complained  of  being 
chased  by  wild  horses. 

Cook  (1975)  concluded  that  the  Wild  Horse  and  Burro  Act  was 
short  sighted.   The  bill  essentially  has  no  control  measures 
and  as  a  result  wild  horses  will  continue  to  increase  to  the 
detriment  of  the  resource;  the  bill  should  be  amended  to  permit 
the  use  of  aircraft  by  the  agencies  for  roundups  and  control; 
the  excess  horses  cannot  be  sold  or  given  away;  and  there  is 
no  provision  to  permit  complete  removal  of  horses  from  some 
areas  of  unsuitable  year-round  habitat. 

Advantages  of  wild  horses.   There  are  certain  advantages 
to  having  wild  horses  on  the  range.   Due  to  their  inefficient 
digestive  system  they  aid  in  spreading  plants  by  the  distribu- 
tion of  feces  that  contain  viable  seeds.   Their  feces  distri- 
bution also  fertilizes  the  soil  and  aids  germination  of  seeds. 
Horses  aid  livestock  and  other  herbivores  by  breaking  ice 
covering  water  holes  in  the  winter  and  help  all  animals  by 
breaking  trails  through  snow.   They  provide  an  esthetic  value 
to  the  western  range  that  has  never  been  exceeded  by  any  other 
animal  (Dobie,  1952;  McKnight,  1959;  Ryden,  1970).   Horses 


53 


graze  very  selectively  and  can  utilize  grasses  too  coarse  for 
most  other  domestic  animals  (Stoddart  e_t  ajL,  1975).   The  removal 
of  coarse  forage  material  exposes  finer,  more  succulent  feed  for 
other  herbivores.   Frei  (1975)  claims  that  wild  horses  use 
range  lands  more  efficiently  than  cattle.   They  travel  much 
greater  distances  in  search  of  forage  and  do  not  excessively 
utilize  vegetation  near  water  because  they  do  not  remain  near 
watering  sources  for  long  periods  like  cattle. 

Disadvantages  of  wild  horses.   McKnight  (1959)  listed 
about  a  dozen  problems  associated  with  wild  horse  from  respond- 
ents to  his  questionnaire.   Heading  the  list  was  competition 
with  livestock  for  forage  and  water.   Other  problems  were  com- 
petition with  big  game,  overgrazing,  trampling,  heavy  grazing 
during  the  spring,  leading  tame  horses  into  the  wilds,  destroy- 
ing range  improvements,  tearing  down  fences,  excluding  other 
animals  from  water,  molesting  livestock,  roiling  or  dirtying 
water  holes,  and  breaking  into  cropland  and  grazing.   Stoddart 
et  al  (1975)  states  that  horses  constantly  seek  fresh  feed  and 
are  capable  of  cropping  forage  very  closely  because  they  have 
both  upper  and  lower  incisor  teeth.   When  horses  are  confined 
in  small  pastures  no  other  animal  can  match  their  impact. 

Research  needs.   All  aspects  of  wild  horse  ecology  need 
research,  and  so  it  is  difficult  to  assign  priorities.   However, 
if  management  of  wild  horses  is  to  be  meaningful  to  the  land 
managers  in  the  near  future,  it  would  appear  that  research  into 
year-round  habitat  requirements  would  assume  great  importance. 
Also  desperately  needed  are  data  on  population  dynamics  and 
competition  with  other  animals. 

Current  research.   At  present  neither  the  Department  of 
Agriculture  nor  of  the  Interior  are  funding  any  research  on 
wild  horses.   Eastern  Montana  College  at  Billings,  Montana, 
and  Colorado  State  University  at  Fort  Collins,  Colorado,  are 
presently  engaged  in  wild  horse  research  that  is  privately 
funded.   Results  have  not  as  yet  been  published. 

Designated  wild  horse  ranges.   In  1962  the  Secretary  of 
the  Interior  established  a  435,000-acre  refuge  for  wild  horses 
in  southern  Nevada.   The  area  is  northwest  of  Las  Vegas  in  the 
northeast  corner  of  Nellis  Air  Force  Base. 

In  1968  the  Secretary  of  the  Interior  established  a  31,000-acre 
wild  horse  range  in  the  Pryor  Mountains  of  south-central 
Montana.   The  area  adjoins  the  Wyoming  state  line  and  the 
Bighorn  Canyon  National  Recreation  Area. 


54 


Legislation  concerning  wild  horses  and  burros.   Congress 
has  passed  two  Federal  laws  to  protect  wild  horses.   Public  Law 
86-234,  passed  in  1959,  makes  it  illegal  to  use  aircraft  or 
motorized  vehicles  to  capture  or  kill  wild  horses.   Public  Law 
92-195,  passed  in  1971,  places  wild  horses  and  burros  roaming 
on  national  resource  lands  under  the  jurisdiction  of  the  Secre- 
taries of  the  Interior  and  of  Agriculture  Departments  for  pro- 
tection, management  and  control.   It  provides  a  penalty  for 
harassing,  capturing,  killing,  or  selling  wild  horses,  and 
prohibits  the  processing  of  wild  horses  into  any  commercial 
product.   The  maximum  penalty  consists  of  a  fine  of  $2,000  and 
imprisonment  for  one  year.   The  Act  of  1971  provides  for  the 
establishment  of  an  advisory  board  to  make  recommendations  on 
the  management  and  protection  of  wild  horses  and  burros. 

Under  the  laws  of  the  various  states,  wild  horses  are  not 
recognized  as  game  animals  or  wildlife.   They  are  considered 
as  "estrays"  or  abandoned  animals  and  are  not  included  under 
the  provisions  of  the  Taylor  Grazing  Act. 

Cooperative  agreements  for  the  protection  and  management  of 
wild  horses  and  burros  are  authorized  between  the  Secretaries 
of  Interior  or  Agriculture  and  state  and  local  government 
agencies  and  with  other  landowners. 

Organizations  concerned  with  the  welfare  of  wild  horses.   The 
inclusion  of  all  the  people,  groups,  animal  welfare  organizations 
or  others  that  have  aided  and  supported  the  wild  horse  cause  would 
be  extremely  lengthy.   Therefore,  the  organizations  listed  below 
include  only  those  that  are  directly  and  exclusively  concerned 
with  the  preservation  and  welfare  of  the  wild  horse. 

1.  American  Horse  Protection  Association 
Washington,  D.  C.   20007 

2.  American  Mustang  Association 
Phoenix,  Arizona 

3.  Canadian  Wildhorse  Society 
Richmond,  British  Columbia 

4.  International  Society  for  the  Protection  of  Mustangs 

and  Burros 
Reno ,  Nevada 

5.  National  Mustang  Association 
Newcastle,  Utah  84756 


55 


6.  Spanish  Barb  Wild  Horse  Research  Farm 
248  N.  Main  Street 

Porterville,  California  93257 

7.  The  Spanish  Mustang  Registry 
Oshoto,  Wyoming 

8.  Wild  Horse  Organized  Assistance 
Reno ,  Nevada 


Glossary.   All  modern  horse  breeds  are  called  either  hot, 
cold,  or  warm  bloods.   The  hot  bloods,  small,  swift  and  tempera- 
mental developed  from  the  Arab  horse.   The  cold  blood,  larger, 
more  placid,  developed  in  northern  Europe  during  the  middle 
ages  to  carry  armored  knights.   The  warm  bloods  are  a  mixture 
of  the  two  (Ryden,  1970). 

Hand  -  the  system  of  measuring  a  horse's  height.   A  hand  is 
four  inches  or  the  width  of  an  adult  man's  hand  across  the 
thumb.   The  term  dates  back  to  ancient  times  (Howard,  1965). 

Mustang  -  a  corruption  of  the  Spanish  word  Mesteno,  which  refers 
to  an  animal  that  belongs  to  everyone  and  not  to  any  particular 
person.   The  term  was  first  applied  to  horses  that  had  escaped 
and  became  wild  in  the  Southwest.   In  a  short  time  all  feral 
and  semi- feral  horses  were  referred  to  as  mustangs.   The  name 
was  also  applied  to  certain  domesticated  horses.   When  feral 
horses  were  caught  and  broken,  the  word  also  came  to  mean  cow 
pony  (McKnight,  1959). 

Cayuse  -  the  term  applied  to  any  horses  that  resembled  Indian 
stock  or  to  cow  ponies  that  were  wild.   The  name  originated 
from  the  Cayuse  Indians  of  Idaho  (a  tribe  now  extinct)  who 
were  known  for  the  numbers  of  horses  they  possessed  (Dobie, 
1952). 

Estray  -  "any  steer,  heifer,  bull,  stag,  cow,  calf,  horse,  mare, 
gelding,  or  colt  not  wearing  a  brand  (Ryden,  1970). 


56 


APPENDIX  1 
Labeled  Points  or  Parts  of  a  Horse 


O 

X 


0 

i 

d 

a 

2 
O 

a. 

Q 

ID 

_A 
UJ 


a. 

/  o 

I 

5 

U 

u. 

Ifc 

p 

0 

o 

V- 

3 

A 

v-» 

L 

Oc2 

57 


APPENDIX  2 
Three  Types  of  Lumbar  Vertebrae  Found  in  Horses 


'.  O 


vr> 


Z 

£ 

Ml 

a 

a 

IL 

o 


58 


APPENDIX  3 

Coat  Colors  in  Horses 

Adapted  from  Gremmel,  1939 


A.  Basic  Colors 

I.   Black  -  body  color  is  true  black  (disregarding  weathering) 

1.  Black  -  true  black  without  light  areas 

2.  Seal  brown  -  black  with  light  areas  to  include 
muzzle,  under  eyes,  flank,  and  inside  of  upper  legs, 
termed  "light  points." 

II.   Bay  -  shades  from  tan  to  brown,  with  black  mane  and 
tail  and  often  with  black  lower  legs: 

1.  Mahogany  bay  -  the  brown  shades  of  bay,  often 
called  brown 

2.  Blood  bay  -  the  red  shades  of  bay 

3.  Sandy  bay  -  the  light  shades  of  bay 

III.   Chestnut  -  shades  from  yellow  gold  to  dark  brown,  mane 
and  tail  not  black  but  approximately  the  color  of 
the  body: 

1.  Liver  chestnut  -  the  dark  shades,  some  appearing 
dark  brown  with  an  auburn  hue 

2.  Sorrel  -  the  red  shades;  sometimes  in  the  lighter 
shades  the  mane  and  tail  are  of  the  color  described 
as  "crushed  strawberry." 

IV.   Ysabella  -  a  color  group  having  flax  or  silver  manes 
and  tails: 

1.  Red  ysabella  -  red  sorrel-like;  flax  mane  and 
tail 

2.  Palomino  -  a  golden  yellow;  silver  mane  and  tail 

3.  Pseudo-albino  -  very  light  cream  to  white;  silver 
mane  and  tail;  often  have  "glass"  or  blue  eyes 

B.  Color  Patterns 

I.   Gray  -  causes  gradual  displacement  of  colored  hair  by 
white  hair  as  age  advances: 

1.  Iron  or  steel  gray  -  usually  a  high  percentage  of 
colored  hair,  indicating  a  young  animal 

2.  Dapple  gray  -  having  the  colored  hair  in  such 
distribution  as  to  give  a  dappled  effect 

3.  White  -  almost  devoid  of  colored  hair 


59 


II.   At  certain  ages  gray,  black  roan,  and  gray  roan  are 
practically  indistinguishable,  but  true  genetic 
differences  exist.   Gray  is  foaled  solid  color;  any 
roan  is  foaled  roan;  and  gray  roan  whitens  with  age 
the  same  as  does  gray. 

Roan,  a  more  or  less  uniform  mixture  of  colored  and 
white  hairs,  occurs  in  a  number  of  combinations: 

1.  Black  roan  -  black  and  white  hairs  mixed,  usually 
called  "blue." 

2.  Blue  roan  -  usually  described  as  black  and  white 
hairs  mixed,  but  almost  invariably  having  some 
red  hairs. 

3.  Bay  or  red  roan  -  roaned  bay. 

4.  Chestnut  or  strawberry  roan  -  roaned  chestnut. 

5.  Paint  roan  -  roaning  imposed  on  the  colored  areas 
of  paint. 

6.  Dun  roan  -  roan  in  combination  with  the  dun  factor 

7.  Gray  roan  -  roan  in  combination  with  the  gray 
factor. 

III.   Dun  -  always  with  dorsal  stripe;  often  zebra  stripes 
on  legs  and  transverse  stripe  over  withers  and 
shoulders;  coat  appears  diluted: 

1.  Mouse  dun  -  dun  imposed  on  black,  seal  brown, 
dark  mahogany  bay,  and  dark  liver  chestnut,  giving 
a  smoky  effect. 

2.  Buckskin  dun  -  dun  imposed  on  blood  and  sandy  bay. 

3.  Claybank  dun  -  dun  imposed  on  sorrel. 

IV.   Paint  or  pied  -  irregular  colored  and  white  areas: 

1.  Piebald  -  white  and  black 

2.  Skewbald  -  white  and  any  color  other  than  black 

Gray,  roan,  dun,  and  pied  or  paint  may  be  in  any 
combination. 


60 


APPENDIX  4 

Movement  on  Land 

Adapted  from  Tricker  and  Tricker,  1966 


Sb. 


£T23 


Three  Gaits  of  the  Horse,  Drawn  from  Cine  Film 


61 


APPENDIX  5 

Evolution  of  the  Horse:   Teeth,  Skulls  and  Feet 

Adapted  from  World  of  Wildlife 


-2. 


14 

Z 

o 

-1 

(X 


o 

-4 
a. 


0) 

IE 

Br — a^cQ  i 


sr=r^D 


^^ 


4} 

o 

X 

LL 
O 

O 


O 

> 
01 


62 


APPENDIX  6 

Plant  Species  Comprising  at  Least  2%   or  More  of  the  Diet 
of  Wild  Horses  in  the  Southwest  Desert  Vegetation  Types 
Adapted  from  Hansen,  1975 


Scientific  Name 


Common  Name 


Agropyron  spp. 
Bouteloua  spp. 
Hilaria  mutica 
Koelaria  cristata 
Leptochloa  dubia 
Muhlenbergia  spp. 
Setaria  macrostachya 
Sporobolus  spp. 
Atriplex  spp. 
Prosopsis  julif lora 
Salsola  spp. 


wheatgrasses 

grama  grasses 

Tobosa  grass 

prairie  junegrass 

green  spangletop  grass 

muhly  grass 

plains  bristlegrass 

dropseed  grass 

saltbush 

mesquite 

russian  thistle 


Plant  Species  Comprising  at  Least  2%  or  More  of  the  Diet 
of  Wild  Horses  for  the  Foothills  and  Northern  Desert  Shrub 
Vegetation  Types  in  Western  States 


Agropyron  spp. 
Bromus  spp. 
Carex  spp. 
Juncus  spp. 
Elymus  spp. 
Festuca  spp. 
Koelaria  cristata 
Oryzopsis  hymenoides 
Poa  spp. 
Stipa  spp. 
Allium  spp. 
Amelanchier  spp. 
Artemisia  frigida 
Artemisia  spp. 
Cercocarpus  spp. 
Chrysothamus  spp. 
Eriogonum  spp. 
Eurotia  lanata 
Leptodactylon  spp. 
Lupinus  spp. 
Phlox  spp. 


wheatgrasses 

brome  grass 

sedges 

rushes 

wildrye  grass 

fescue  grass 

prairie  junegrass 

Indian  ricegrass 

bluegrass 

needle  and  thread  grass 

wild  onion 

service  berry 

fringed  sagewort 

sagebrushes 

mountain  mahogany 

rabbitbrush 

buckwheat 

winterfat 

prickly  phlox 

lupine 

phlox 


63 


APPENDIX  7 

The  Spread  of  the  Horse  to  the  Western  World 

Adapted  from  Smith,  1969 


The  spread  of  the  horse 


5AM  RAFfttl 

Ml^SlOtJ 
DE  CAEME.L 


Ml^lO 


64 


to  the  western  world 


eoi/rE£ 


LITERATURE  CITED 


Allee,  W.  C.   1931.   Animal  Aggregations:   A  Study  in  General 
Sociology.   University  of  Chicago  Press,  Chicago.   431  p. 

Anonymous.   1971.   Family:   Equidae.   World  of  Wildlife.  1(5): 
86-88. 

Anonymous.   1974.   Description  of  the  Barb.   Spanish  Barb  Wild 
Horse  Research  Farm.   Porterville,  California.   2  p., 
mimeographed . 

Antonius,  0.   1937.   Uber  Herdenbildung  und  Paarungs  eigentum- 
lichkeiten  der  Einhufer.   Z.  Tierpsychol. ,  1:259-289. 

Beebe,  B.  F.  and  J.  R.  Johnson.   1964.   American  Wild  Horses. 
David  McKay,  New  York,  180  p. 

Bell,  R.  H.  V.   1970.   The  use  of  the  herb  layer  by  grazing 
ungulates  in  the  Serengeti,  p.  111-124.   In:   A.  Watson, 
ed. ,  Animal  Populations  in  Relation  to  their  Food  Resources. 
Blackwell,  Oxford. 

.   1971.   A  grazing  ecosystem  in  the  Serengeti. 


Scientific  American.  225(1) :86-93. 

Berliner,  V.  R.   1969.   The  estrous  cycle  of  the  mare,  p.  267. 
In:   H.  H.  Cole  and  P.  T.  Cupps,  eds.,  Reproduction  in 
Domestic  Animals.   Academic  Press,  New  York. 

Blakeslee,  J.  K.   1974.   Mother- Young  Relationships  and  Related 
Behavior  Among  Free-Ranging  Appaloosa  Horses.   M.  S.  Thesis, 
Idaho  State  University.   113  p. 

Bone,  Jesse  F.   1964.   The  age  of  the  horse.   Southwest  Veter- 
inarian. 17(4):269-272. 

Boone,  A.  R.   1933.   The  wild  herd  passes.   Travel.  60(4):20-23, 
56. 

Boughey,  A.  S.   1968.   Ecology  of  Populations.   Macmillan,  New 
York.   135  p. 

Burkhardt,  D. ,  W.  Schleidt,  and  H.  Altner.   1967.   Signals  in 
the  Animal  World.   McGraw-Hill,  New  York.   150  p. 

Burkhardt,  J.   1947.   Transition  from  anestrus  in  the  mare  and 
the  effects  of  artificial  lighting.   Journal  of  Agricultural 
Science.  37:64-68. 


66 


Burt,  W.  H.   1943.   Territoriality  and  home  range  concepts  as 
applied  to  mammals.   Journal  of  Mammalogy.  24:346-352. 

Castle,  W.  E.   1954.   Coat  color  inheritance  in  horses  and  in 
other  mammals.   Genetics.  39(l):35-44. 

Catlin,  George.   1838.   Commanche  horses,  p.  145-150.  In:   J.  F. 
Dobie,  M.  C.  Boatright  and  H.  H.  Ransom,  Mustangs  and  Cow 
Horses.   1965,  2nd  ed.  Southern  Methodist  University  Press, 
Dallas. 

Clegg,  M.  T.  and  W.  F.  Ganong.   1969.   Environmental  factors 
affecting  reproduction,  p.  473-488.   In:   H.  H.  Cole  and 
P.  T.  Cupps ,  eds.,  Reproduction  in  Domestic  Animals.   Academic 
Press,  New  York. 

Collias,  N.  E.  1970.   Aggressive  behavior  among  vertebrate 

animals,  p.  14-39.   In:   C.  H.  Southwick,  ed. ,  Animal 

Aggression:  Selected  Readings.   Van  Nostrand  Reinhold, 
New  York. 

Cook,  C.  W.  and  L.  F.  Harris.   1968.   Nutritive  Value  of  Seasonal 
Ranges.   Utah  State  University  Agricultural  Experiment 
Station  Bulletin  #472.   Logan,  Utah.   55  p. 

Cook,  C.  W.   197  5.   Wild  horses  and  burros:   a  new  management 
problem.   Rangeman ' s  Journal.  2(1):19-21. 

Cook,  J.  H.   1919.   Wild  horses  of  the  plains.   Natural  History. 
19:104-110. 

Crain,  Carolyn.   1973.   Saving  the  Symbol  of  the  West:   The 
Wild  Horses.   Unpublished  bibliographic  essay.   18  p. 

Denhardt,  R.  M.   1948.   The  Horse  of  the  Americas.   University 
of  Oklahoma  Press,  Norman,  Oklahoma.   286  p. 

Dobie,  J.  F.   1952.   The  Mustangs.   Little,  Brown  and  Company, 
Boston.   376  p. 

Edwards,  Gladys  Brown.   1970.   The  long  and  short  of  it. 
Arabian  Horse  World.  10:87-94. 

Ehrenfeld,  D.  W.   1972.   Conserving  Life  on  Earth.   Oxford 
University  Press,  New  York.   360  p. 

Ensminger,  M.  E.   1951.   Horse  Husbandry.   Interstate  Printers 
and  Publishers,  Danville,  Illinois.   336  p. 

Estes,  R.  D.   1974.   Zebras  offer  clues  to  the  way  wild  horses 
once  lived.   Smithsonian.  5(8) : 100-107 . 


67 


Feist,  J.  D.   1971.   Behavior  of  Feral  Horses  in  the  Pryor 
Mountain  Wild  Horse  Range.   M.  S.  Thesis,  University  of 
Michigan.   129  p. 

Frei,  Milton.   1975.   Personal  communication.   U.  S.  Department 
of  the  Interior,  Bureau  of  Land  Management,  Denver,  Colorado, 

Glover,  J.  and  D.  W.  Duthie.   1958.   The  nutritive  ratio  crude 
protein  relationships  in  ruminant  and  non-ruminant  digestion, 
Journal  of  Agricultural  Science.  50:227-229. 

Gremmel,  F.   1939.   Coat  color  in  horses.   Journal  of  Heredity. 
30(10) :437-445. 

Hafez,  E.  S.  E.,  M.  Williams  and  S.  Wierzbowski.   1962.   The 

behaviour  of  horses,  p.  370-396.   In:   E.  S.  E.  Hafez,  The 

Behaviour  of  Domestic  Animals.   Williams  and  Wilkins, 
Baltimore. 

.   1969.   The  behaviour  of  horses,  p.  391-416.   In: 

E.  S.  E.  Hafez,  The  Behaviour  of  Domestic  Animals.   Williams 
and  Wilkins,  Baltimore. 

Haines,  Francis.   1971.   Horses  in  America.   Thomas  Y.  Crowell, 
New  York.   213  p. 

Hall,  R.   1972.   Wild  Horse:   Biology  and  Alternatives  for 

Management,  Pryor  Mountain  Wild  Horse  Range.   Bureau  of  Land 
Management,  Billings  District.   67  p. 

1974.   Wild  Horse  Capture  Techniques.   Bureau  of 


Land  Management,  Utah  State  Office,  Salt  Lake  City,  Utah. 
16  p. 

^ .   1975.   Personal  communication,  U.  S.  Department 


of  the  Interior,  Bureau  of  Land  Management,  Salt  Lake  City, 
Utah. 

Hanauer,  Elsie.   1973.   The  Science  of  Equine  Feeding.   A.  S. 
Barnes,  New  York.   78  p. 

Hansen,  R.  M.  1975.  Personal  communication.  Department  of 
Range  Science.  Colorado  State  University,  Fort  Collins, 
Colorado. 

Howard,  R.  W.   1965.   The  Horse  in  America.   Follett,  Chicago. 
289  p. 

Howell,  C.  E.   1945.   Vertebrae  in  Arabian  horses.   California 
Cultivator.  Vol.  92:520.   Sept.  29. 


68 


Hoyt,  H.  S.   1886.   Mustanging  on  the  staked  plains,  p.  96-101. 
In:   J.  F.  Dobie,  M.  C.  Boatright  and  H.  H.  Ransom,  eds., 
Mustangs  and  Cow  Horses.   1965,  2nd  ed.  Southern  Methodist 
University  Press,  Dallas. 

Huffman,  W.  T. ,  E.  A.  Moran,  and  W.  Binns.   1956.   Poisonous 
Plants.   In:   Yearbook  of  Agriculture  Animal  Diseases. 
U.  S.  Department  of  Agriculture,  Washington,  D.  C.   118-130. 

James,  T.  J.   1823.   Comanche  Horses,   pp.  143-144.   In: 
J.  Frank  Dobie,  Mody  C.  Boatright  and  Harry  H.  Ransom, 
Mustangs  and  Cov  Horses.   1965.   2nd  Ed.  Southern  Methodist 
University  Press,  Dallas,  Texas. 

Kleiman,  Devra.   1966.   Scent  marking  in  the  Canidae.   p.  167-177 
In:   P.  A.  Jewell  and  Caroline  Loizos,  eds.  Play,  Explora- 
tion and  Territory  in  Mammals.   Academic  Press,  New  York. 

Klingel,  H.   1965.   Notes  on  the  biology  of  the  plains  zebra, 

Equus  quagga  boehmi.   East  African  Wildlife  Journal.  3:86-88. 

1969.   Reproduction  in  the  plains  zebra,  Equus 


Burchellis  boehmi :   behavior  and  ecological  factors. 
Journal  Reprod.  Fert.,  Suppl.  6:339-345. 

1972.   Social  behaviour  of  African  Equidae. 


Zoological  Africa.  7 (1) : 175-185. 

Linsdale,  J.  M.  and  P.  Q.  Tomich.   1953.   A  Herd  of  Mule  Deer, 
Odocoileus  hemionus  columbianus.   A  record  of  observations 
made  on  the  Hastings  Natural  History  Reservation.   Berkeley 
and  Los  Angeles. 

Loizos,  Caroline.   1966.   Play  in  mammals,  p.  1-9.   In:   P.  A. 
Jewell  and  Caroline  Loizos,  eds.,  Play,  Exploration  and 
Territory  in  Mammals.   Academic  Press,  New  York. 

Lorenz,  K.   1963.   On  Aggression.   Marjorie  Kerr  Wilson,  trans. 
Harcourt,  Brace  and  World,  New  York.   306  p. 

Lyall-Watson,  M.   1964.   The  Ethology  of  Food  Hoarding  in 
Mammals .   Ph.D.  Dissertation,  University  of  London. 

Mathews,  H.  L.   1964.   Overt  fighting  in  mammals,  p.  23-32.   In: 
J.  D.  M'Carthy  and  F.  J.  Ebling,  eds.,  The  Natural  History 
of  Aggression.   Academic  Press,  New  York. 

McKnight,  T.  L.   1959.   The  feral  horse  in  Anglo-America. 
Geographical  Review.   49:506-525. 


69 


Nishikawa,  Y. ,  T.  Svjie  and  N.  Haracla.   1952.   35  Animal  Breed- 
ing Abstracts.  22:103.   Bulletin  National  Institute  of 
Agricultural  Science  Series  G3.   1954. 

Odberg,  F.  0.   1972.   An  Interpretation  of  Pawing  by  the  Horse 
(Equus  caballus  L.):   Displacement  Activity  and  Original 
Functions.   Revised  part  of  a  B.S.  Thesis,  State  University 
of  Gent,  Belgium.   12  p. 

Odum,  E.  P.  1970.  Fundamentals  of  Ecology.  3rd  ed.  Saunders, 
Philadelphia.   574  p. 

Pelligrini,  S.  W.   1971.   Home  Range,  Territoriality  and  Move- 
ment Patterns  of  Wild  Horses  in  the  Wassuk  Range  of  Western 
Nevada.   M.S.  Thesis,  University  of  Nevada,  Reno.   39  p. 

Roby,  T.  0.  and  L.  0.  Mott.   1956.   Diseases  and  parasites 

affecting  horses  and  mules,   p.  531-533.   In:   Yearbook  of 
Agriculture  Animal  Diseases.   U.  S.  Department  of  Agriculture, 
Washington,  D.C. 

Ruckebusch,  U. ,  P.  Barbey  and  P.  Guillemot.  1970.  Les  etats  de 
Sommeil  chez  le  Cheval  (Equus  caballus) .  C.  R.  Seances  Soc. 
Biol.  Filiales.   164(3) :658-665. 

Ryden,  Hope.   1970.   America's  Last  Wild  Horses.   Dutton,  New 
York.   311  p. 

Schenkel,  R.   1947.   Expression  Studies  of  Wolves.   Behaviour. 
1:81-129. 


Schneider,  K 
198. 


200-226. 


297. 


M.   1930.   Das  Flehmen  I.   Zool.  Gart.  Lpz.  3:183- 

1931.   Das  Flehmen  II.   Zool.  Gart.  Lpz.  4:349-364. 
1932a.   Das  Flehmen  III.   Zool.  Gart.  Lpz.  5: 

1932b.   Das  Flehmen  IV.   Zool.  Gart.  Lpz.  5:287- 


Schwartz,  B.   1949.   A  kingdom  for  wild  horses.   Nature  Magazine. 
42(1):8-12,  50. 

Scott,  J.  P.   1958.   Aggression.   University  of  Chicago  Press, 
Chicago,  111.   149  p. 


70 


Simpson,  G.  G.   1951.   Horses;  The  Story  of  the  Horse  Family  in 
the  Modern  World  and  through  Sixty  Million  Years  of  History. 
Oxford  University  Press,  New  York.   247  p. 

Smith,  B.   1969.   The  Horse  in  the  West.   World  Publishing  Com- 
pany, New  York.   255  p. 

Smith,  C.  H.   1841.   Feral  horses  of  America,  p.  173-185.   In: 
C.  H.  Smith,  The  Naturalist's  Library,  Mammalia  Vol.  XII. 
Horses .   W.  H.  Lizars,  Edinburgh. 

Smythe,  R.  H.   1966.   The  Mind  of  the  Horse.   Stephen  Greene 
Press,  Brattleboro,  Vermont.   123  p. 

Southwick,  C.  H.   1970.   Conflict  and  violence  in  animal  societies, 
p.  1-13.   In:   C.  H.  Southwick,  ed. ,  Animal  Aggression: 
Selected  Readings.   Van  Nostrand  Reinhold,  New  York. 

Stecher,  R.  M.   1962.   Lateral  facets  and  lateral  joints  in  the 
lumbar  spine  of  the  horse.   American  Journal  of  Veterinary 
Research.  23(96) : 934-947 . 

Stoddart,  L.  A.,  A.  D.  Smith,  T.  W.  Box.   1975.   Range  Manage- 
ment.  McGraw-Hill.   New  York.   532  p. 

Tricker,  R.  A.  R.  and  B.  J.  K.  Tricker.   1967.   The  Science  of 
Movement.   American  Elsevier,  New  York.   284  p. 

Trumler,  E.   1958.   Beobachtungen  and  den  Bohm-zebras  des  Georg 
von  Opel-Friegeheges  fur  Tierforschung.  I.   Das  Paarungvser- 
halten.   Saugetierk  Mitt.  6:1-48. 

Tyler,  S.   197  2.   The  Behavior  and  Social  Organization  of  New 
Forest  Ponies.   Animal  Behavior  Monographs.   5(2):84-196. 

U.  S.  Department  of  the  Interior,  Bureau  of  Land  Management. 
1973.   Environmental  Impact  Statement  and  Proposed  Manage- 
ment Regulations  for  Wild  Free-Roaming  Horses  and  Burros. 
133  p. 

.   1974.   Livestock  Grazing  Management  on  National 

Resource  Lands.   Final  Environmental  Impact  Statement.  3  vols. 

U.  S.  Department  of  the  Interior,  U.  S.  Department  of  Agriculture, 
June,  1974.   A  Report  to  Congress  by  the  Secretary  of  the 
Interior  and  the  Secretary  of  Agriculture  on  Administration 
of  the  Wild  Free-Roaming  Horse  and  Burro  Act:   P.L.  92-195. 
58  p. 


71 


U.  S.  Department  of  the  Interior,  Bureau  of  Land  Management  and 
U.  S.  Department  of  Agriculture,  Forest  Service.   1973. 
Proceedings  of  the  National  Advisory  Board  for  Wild  Free- 
Roaming  Horses  and  Burros.   7  p.   14  appendices. 

Waring,  G.  H.   1970.   Perinatal  behavior  of  foals  (Equus  caballus) 
Paper  presented  at  50th  Annual  Meeting  of  the  American 
Society  of  Mammalogists.   June  18,  1970.   College  Station, 
Texas. 

1970.   Primary  socialization  of  the  foal  (Equus 


caballus) .   Paper  presented  at  the  Animal  Behavior  Society 
at  the  21st  Annual  AIBS  meeting.   Indiana  University, 
Bloomington,  Indiana,  August  29,  1970. 

.   1971.   Sounds  of  the  horse  (Equus  caballus). 


Paper  presented  to  the  Ecological  Society  of  America.   22nd 
Annual  American  Institute  of  Biological  Sciences  meeting, 
Colorado  State  University,  Fort  Collins,  Colorado.   Sept.  2, 
1971. 

Worcester,  D.  E.   1945.   Spanish  horses  among  the  plains  tribes. 
Pacific  Historical  Review.   14:409-417. 

World  of  Wildlife.   1971.   Vol.  1,  part  5.   Orbis  Publishing 
Ltd. ,  London. 

Wyman,  Walker  D.   1945.   The  Wild  Horse  of  the  West.   Caxton 
Printers,  Caldwell,  Idaho.   348  p. 

Young,  S.  P.  and  E.  A.  Goldman.   1946.   The  Puma,  Mysterious 

American  Cat.   American  Wildlife  Institute,  Washington,  D.C. 
358  p. 

Zeeb,  K.   1959.   Die  Unterlegenheitsgebarde'  des  noch  nicht 

ausgewachsenen  Pferdes,  (Equus  caballus) .   Z.  Tierpsychol . , 
16:489-496. 

Ziswiler,  V.   1967.   Extinct  and  Vanishing  Animals.   Revised 
English  Edition  by  F.  and  P.  Bunnell.   Springer-Verlag. 


ral  Cerr 


72