For Reference NOT TO BE TAKEN FROM THIS ROOM JF&t^ fa &« -h J~^^ ' AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY HENRY P. BOWDITCH, M. D. JOHN G. CURTIS, M. D. HENRY H. DONALDSON, Ph. D. W. H. HOWELL, Ph.D., M.D. FREDERIC S. LEE, Ph.D. WARREN P. LOMBARD, M.D. GRAHAM LUSK, Ph.D.,F.R.S. (Edin.j W. T. PORTER, M.D. EDWARD T. REICHERT, M.D. HENRY SEW ALL, Ph.D., M.D. ORS. TASKER & TASKER OSTEOPATHIC PHYSICIANS 526-529 Auditorium Bldg, Le« Angeles, Cal. WILLIAM H. HOWELL, Ph.D., M.D. Professor of Physiology in the Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, Md. EDITED BY SECOND EDITION, REVISED Vol. II. MUSCLE AND NERVE; CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM; THE SPECIAL SENSES; SPECIAL MUSCULAR MECHANISMS; REPRODUCTION PHILADELPHIA AND LONDON W. B. SAUNDERS & COMPANY 1901 CM Copyright, 1900, By W. B. SAUNDERS & COMPANY PRESS OF W. B. SAUNDERS 4 COMPANY CONTRIBUTORS TO VOLUME II. HENRY P. BOWDITCH, M.D., Professor of Physiology in the Harvard Medical School. HENRY H. DONALDSON, Ph.D., Professor of Neurology in the University of Chicago. FREDERIC S. LEE, Ph.D., Adjunct Professor of Physiology in Columbia University (College of Physicians and Surgeons). WARREN P. LOMBARD, M. D., Professor of Physiology in the University of Michigan. HENRY SEWALL, Ph. D., M. D., Professor of Physiology in the Denver College of Medicine, Medical Department of the University of Denver. PREFACE TO THE SECOND EDITION. Advantage has been taken of the necessity of issuing a second edition of the American Text-Book of Physiology to alter somewhat its general arrangement. The hook has proved to be successful, and for the most part has met only with kindly and encouraging criticisms from those who have made use of it. Many teachers, however, have suggested that the size of the book, when issued in a single volume, has constituted to some extent an inconvenience when regarded from the standpoint of a student's textbook that may be needed daily for consultation in the lecture-room or the labora- torv. It has been thought best, therefore, to issue the present edition in two volumes, with the hope that the book may thereby be made more serviceable to those for whose aid it was especially written. This change in the appearance of the book has necessitated also some alteration in the arrangement of the sections, the part upon the Physiology of Nerve and Muscle being transferred to the second volume, so as to bring it into its natural relations with the Physiology of the Central Nervous System. The actual amount of material in the book remains substantially the same as in the first edition, although, naturally, very many changes have been made. Even in the short time that has elapsed since the appearance of the first edition there has been much progress in physiology, as the result of the constant activity of experimenters in this and the related sciences in all parts of the world, and an effort has been made by the various contributors to keep pace with this progress. Statements and theories that have been shown to be wrong or improbable have been eliminated, and the new facts discovered and the newer points of view have been incorporated so far as possible. Such changes are found scattered throughout the book. ' The only distinctly new matter that can be referred to specifically is found in the section upon the Central Nervous System, and in a short section upon the modern ideas and nomenclature of physical chemistry, with reference especially to the processes of osmosis and diffusion, 'flic section dealing with the Central Nervous System has been recast in large part, with the intention of making it more suitable to the actual needs of medical students ; while a brief presen- tation of some of the elementary conceptions of physical chemistry seems to be necessary at the present time, owing to the large part that these views are taking in current discussions in physiological and medical literature. The index has been revised thoroughly ami considerably amplified, a table of contents has been added to each volume, and numerous new figures have been introduced. August, 1900. PKEFACE. The collaboration of several teachers in the preparation of an elementary text-book of physiology is unusual, the almost invariable rule heretofore having been for a single author to write the entire book. It does not seem desirable to attempt a discussion of the relative merits and demerits of the two plans, since the method of collaboration is untried in the teaching of physi- ology, and there is therefore no basis for a satisfactory comparison. It is a fact, however, that many teachers of physiology in this country have not been altogether satisfied with the text-books at their disposal. Some of the more successful older books have not kept pace with the rapid changes in modern physiology, while few, if any, of the newer books have been uniformly satis- factory in their treatment of all parts of this many-sided science. Indeed, the literature of experimental physiology is so great that it would seem to be almost impossible for any one teacher to keep thoroughly informed on all topics. This fact undoubtedly accounts for some of the defects of our present text-books, and it is hoped that one of the advantages derived from the col- laboration method is that, owing to the less voluminous literature to be consulted, each author has been enabled to base his elementary account upon a comprehensive knowledge of the part of the subject assigned to him. Those who are acquainted with the difficulty of making a satisfactory elementary presentation of the complex and oftentimes unsettled questions of physiology must agree that authoritative statements and generalizations, such as are fre- quently necessary in text-books if they are to leave any impression at all upon the student, are usually trustworthy in proportion to the fulness of informa- tion possessed by the writer. Perhaps the most important advantage which may be expected to follow the use of the collaboration method is that the student gains thereby the point of view of a number of teachers. In a measure he reaps the same benefit as would be obtained by following courses of instruction under different teachers. The different standpoints assumed, and the differences in emphasis laid upon the various lines of procedure, chemical, physical, and anatomical, should give the student a better insight into the methods of the science as it exists PBEFA CE. to-day. A similar advantage may be expected to follow the inevitable over- lapping of the topics assigned to the various contributors, since this has led in many cases to a treatment of the same subject by several writers, who have approached the matter under discussion from slightly varying standpoints, and in a few instances have arrived at slightly different conclusions. In this last respect the book reflects more faithfully perhaps than if written by a single author the legitimate differences of opinion which are held by physi- ologists at present with regard to certain questions, and in so far it fulfils more perfectly its object of presenting in an unprejudiced way the existing state of our knowledge. It is hoped, therefore, that the diversity in method of treatment, which at first sight might seem to be disadvantageous, will prove to be the most attractive feature of the book. In the preparation of the book it has been assumed that the student has previously obtained some knowledge of gross and microscopic anatomy, or is taking courses in these subjects concurrently with his physiology. For this reason no systematic attempt has been made to present details of histology or anatomy, but each author has been left free to avail himself of material of this kind according as he felt the necessity for it in developing the physiolog- ical side. In response to a general desire on the part of the contributors, references to literature have been given in the book. Some of the authors have used these freely, even to the point of giving a fairly complete bibliography of the subject, while others have preferred to employ them only occasionally, where the facts cited are recent or are noteworthy because of their importance or historical interest. References of this character are not usually found in ele- mentary text books, so that a brief word of explanation seems desirable. It has not been supposed that the student will necessarily look up the references or commit to memory the names of the authorities quoted, although it is pos- sible, of course, that individual students may be led to refer occasionally to original sources, and thereby acquire a truer knowledge of the subject. The main result hoped for, however, is a healthful pedagogical influence. It is too often the case that the student of medicine, or indeed the graduate in medicine, regards his text-book as a final authority, losing sight of the fad that such books are mainly compilations from the works of various investigators, and that in all matters in dispute in physiology the final decision must lie made, so far as possible, upon the evidence furnished by experimental work. To enforce this latter idea and to indicate the character and source of the great literature from which the material of the text-book is obtained have been the main reasons for the adoption of the reference system. It is hoped also that the PREFACE. book will be found useful to many practitioners of medicine who may wish to keep themselves in touch with the development of modern physiology. For this class of readers references to literature are not only valuable, but frequently essential, since the limits of a text-book forbid an exhaustive discussion of manv points of interest concerning which fuller information may be desired. The numerous additions which are constantly being made to the literature of physiology and the closely related sciences make it a matter of difficulty to escape errors of statement in any elementary treatment of the subject. It can- not be hoped that this book will be found entirely free from defects of this character, but an earnest effort has been made to render it a reliable repository of the important facts and principles of physiology, and, moreover, to embody in it, so far as possible, the recent discoveries and tendencies which have so characterized the history of this science within the last few years. CONTENTS OF VOLUME I. INTRODUCTION (By W. II. Howell). BLOOD (By W. H. Howell). LYMPH (By W. H. Howell). CIRCULATION (By John G. Curtis and W. T. Porter). SECRETION (By W. II. Howell). CHEMISTRY OF DIGESTION AND NUTRITION (By W. H. Howell). MOVEMENTS OF THE ALIMENTARY CANAL, BLADDER, AND URETER (By W. H. Howell). RESPIRATION (By Edward T. Reichert). ANIMAL HEAT (By Edward T. Reichert). THE CHEMISTRY OF THE ANIMAL BODY (By Graham Lusk). CONTENTS OF VOLUME II. PAGE GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE (By Warren P. Lombard) 17 A. Introduction 17 The general property of contractility, 17 — The movements of amcebje, leucocytes, vorticella, etc., 19 — The general property of Irritability, 20 — The general property of conductivity, 20 — The general distribution of the properties of conductivity and irri- tability, 21. B. Irritability of Muscle and Nerve 23 Definition of various irritants, 23 — The persistence of irritability in excised organs, 24 — Irritability of nerves, 24 — Demonstration of irritability by various forms of stimuli, 25 — The independent irritability of muscle, 25 — The curare experiment to prove independent irritability, 26 — Other proofs of direct irritability of muscle, 27 — Conditions that determine the efficiency of irritants, 28 — Irritating effect of the electrical current, 28 — Description of the apparatus used in electrical stimulation, 29 — Effect of the rate of stimulation, 31 — Du Bois-Reymond's law, 32 — Irritating effect of induced electric currents, 33 — The myogram, 34 — The make and break shocks of the induction current, 35 — kathodal and anodal contractions, 35 — Description of the commutator, 36 — The closing contractions stronger than the opening contractions, 38— The effect of variations in the strength of stimuli, 39— The effect of density of the electrical current, 41 — The spread of the electric current in moist conductors, 41 — The spread of electrostatic charges, 42 — Means of preventing the spread of cur- rent, 44 — The unipolar method of excitation, 45 — The effect of duration of current on its stimulating action, 46 — The effect of the angle at which the current enters, 48 — The effect of the direction of the current (Pfluger's law1, ID — The effect of battery currents on normal human nerves, 51 — The conditions that determine the irritability of nerves and muscles, 55 — The effect of mechanical agencies, 55 — The effect of tem- perature, 56 — The effect of chemicals and drugs, 58 — The effect of the electrical cur- rent on muscle, 61 — The effect of the electrical current on nerve, 62 — Electrotonus, 63 — Effect of rapidity of stimulation, 65 — The effect of varying the normal blood- supply, 66 — The effect of separation from the central nervous system, 69 — The effect of the fatigue of muscles, 70 — The fatigue of nerves, 75 — The effect of use and disuse, 76 — The effect of enforced rest, 77. C. Conductivity 77 The necessity of protoplasmic continuity for conduction, 77 — Isolated conduction in nerve-trunks, 79 — The distribution of the excitation by the branches of a nerve, 80 — conduction in muscles, 80— The transmission of the excitation by means of end- organs, 82 — Conduction in both dh'eetions in muscles, 84 — In nerves, 85 — The rate of conduction, 87 — Transmission of the wave of contraction, 87 — The length of the contraction wave, 88 — The rate of conduction in different kinds of muscles, 89 Elate of conduction in motor nerves, 89 — Rate of conduction in sensory nerves, 91 — The effect of deatli processes on the conduction, 91 — The effect of mechanical conditions on conduction, 92 The effect of temperature on conduction. 92 — The effect of chemicals and drugs on conduction, 93 — The effect of a constant battery current on conduction, 94 — Practical application of the foregoing effect, 95— The relation of conduction to the fatigue of nerves, 95 — Nature of the conduction process (nerve- impulse), 97. D. Contractility 99 Graphic records of simple muscle contractions. 99- The myograph, 100 — The chronograph, 100 The latent period. L02 -Optical properties of muscle daring rest and contraction, 103 The elasticity of muscle. L05 The muscle contraction in dif- ferent muscles, 108 The effect of tension on the curve of contraction, 109 The effect Of rate of excitation on the curve of contraction. 111 Introductory and stair- case contractions, 112 The effect of fatigue fr repeated stimulation, 113 The effect of repeated stimulations on the form of separate contractions, I 1 5 The pro- duction of tetanus by repeated stimulations, 117 — The cause of summation in tetanic contractions, 120 The effect of two excitations following rapidly, i".'i The effect of support on the height of contractions, 122 -The effect of gradually increasing the rate of excitation, 123 Summary of the factors .producing tetanus, 121 The number of stimulations necessary to produce tetanus, 125 The effect of very rapid rates of excitation, 126— Relative intensity of tetanus and single contractions. 126 Con- 11 12 CONTENTS. PAGE tractares and continuous contractions, 127— Contracture following frequent excita- tions, L28 Contracture following single excitations. 129— The effect of fatigue ou contracture, 130 Effect of tlie constanl current on the form of contraction, 131 — The effect of death processes on the form of contraction, 132 — Normal physiological contractions, 132 Muscle sounds, tremors, etc., 132— Comparison of effects of normal and artificial stimulation, 134 — Fatigue of voluntary muscle contractions, 134 — Effect of temperature on muscular contractions, 136 -Effecl of drugs and chemicals upon muscular contractions, 137 — Liberation of energy by the contracting muscle, 13b — Conditions cou trolling the amount of work done by a muscle, L39 — The thermal energy given oil' by a contracting muscle, 1 11 -Muscle-tonus and chemical tonus, 143. E. Ele< rBiCAX Phenomena in Muscle and Nerve 144 Liberation of electrical energy during functional activity, 144 — Description of the galvanometer and capillary electrometer. 145— The current of rest, 117- Theories as to the cause of the current of rest, 1 1> -The current of rest in nerves, 149 Currents of action in muscle, 150- Secondary tetanus, loll- -The diphasic act ion currents, 152 — Relation of the action current to the muscle contraction, 153 — Currents of action in nerves, 153- Gelation of the electrical phenomena of nerves to the physiological pro- cesses, L57. F. Chemistry of Muscle and Nerve 159 The condition of rigor mortis. 159 Conditions influencing the development of rigor mortis, 160 The cause and nature of the contraction of rigor mortis, 161— The chemical changes occurring in rigor mortis, 162 — Rigor caloris, 164 — The constituents of muscle serum, muscle proteids, 166— Nitrogenous extraction of muscle, 166 — The non-nitrogenous constituents of muscle, 167— The gases of muscle, 168 — The cheni- ist rv of nerves, 169. CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM (By Henry H. Donaldson) 171 Introduction 171 The unity of the, central nervous system. 171 — Phenomena in vol ving consciousness, 172 — Growth and organization, 172 — Plan of presentation of the subject, 172. Part I.— Physiology of the Xkrve-cell 173 A. Anatomical Characteristics of the Nerve-celi 173 Form of nerve-cells, 173 — Peculiarities in structure of nerve-cells, 174 — The volume relations of nerve-cells. 17." — Size of nerve-cells iu different animals, 175 — Relation between size and function, 175 — The growth of nerve-cells, 176— Maturing of nerve- cells. 177 — Classification of cells by means of the form of the ax one, 177 — Growth of the branches of the nerve-cell, 179 — Internal structure of the neurones, 179 — Medulla- tion of nerve-fihres, 179— Growth of the medullary sheath in peripheral nerves, 180 — Medullation in the central system, 181 — Changes m the cytoplasm, 182 — Old age of the nerve-cells, 182. B. The Nebve Impulse Within a Single Neurone 183 The nerve-impulse, 183 Direction of the nerve-impulse, 184 — Double pathways for the nerve-impulse. 185 Significance of cell branches, 186 — The generation of nerve- impulses, 1S7 — The rate of discharge of nerve-impulses, 189 — Points at which the nerve-impulse can be aroused, L89— Irritability and conductivity, 189 — Summation of stimuli in nerve-cells. 190. C. Tin; Nutrition of the Nerve-cell 191 Chemical changes in the nerve-cell. 191- Fatigue of the nerve-cell, 191 — Atrophic influences affecting the nerve-cell, 195— Effects of amputations in man on the nerve- cells, 196 Degeneration of nerve elements, 197 — The nutritive control of the neurone, 198 1 >egenera1 ion of the cell-body, 199 -Regeneration of the axone, 199. Part II. — The Physiology of Groi ps of Nerve-cells 202 a. Architects re \ni> Organization of the Central Nervoub System . . . 202 General arrangement of the central nervous system. 202— Arrangement of the cells forming the several groups, 205 Segmentation of the central nervous system, 205 — Relative development of different parts, 206 — The connections between cells, 206 — Theories of t he passage of the nerve- impulses, 207. B. REFLEX Actions 207 The condition-; of stimulation controlling reflex actions, 297 — The diffusion of cen- tral impulses, 208— Simple reflex actions, 208 Influence of location of stimulus on reflexes, 209 Segmental reflex actions, 210- The influence of the strength of stimulus on reflex actions, 210 Continuance of the reflex response. 211 The latent period of reflexes, 211 The summation of stimuli in reflexes, 211- Reflex reactions from frac- tions of the cord, 212 Re Ilex reactions in other vertebrates, 212 Co-ordination of the. liferent impulses iu reflex actions. 211 — Purposeful character of reflex responses, 215 Reflexes in man, 216 Periodic reflexes, 216— Variations in diffusibility in reflexes, 217 — Influence of strychnine on reflexes, 217 -Peripheral diffusion of reflex impulses, 218 — Reflexes in the sympathetic system, 218 Manner of diffusion of impulses in CONTEXTS. 13 PAGE the sympathetic system, 219— Evidence for continuous outgoing impulses from the central nervous system, 220— Rigor mortis as affected by the nervous system. 220 Modification of reflexes by simultaneous and successive afferent impulses, -2,21— Effects of afferent impulses on reflexes, 223— Inhibition of reflexes, 223. C. Reactions Involving the Encephalon 226 The path of afferent impulses in the central nervous system, 226— Degenerations in the spinal cord after hemisection, 228— Physiological observations on afferenl pathways in the central nervous system, 229— The nerves of common sensation. 230- I he nerves of pain and their pathway in the cord, 231— The pathways ol impulses m the cord, 233— The nuclei and courses of the cranial nerves, 230— The pathway ot the libres ot the optic nerve, 238— The pathway of the fibres of the olfactory nerve, 240. D. Localization of Cell-gboups in the Cerebral Cortex 241 The discovery of localization of function in the cortex, 241— Effects of stimulation of the i cortex, 241 Course of the descending impulses, 244— Mapping ol the cortex, 247— The size of the cortical areas, 247— Subdivision of the cortical areas, 247- Sepa- rateness of centres and areas, 248— Multiple control of muscles from the cortex, 250— Cortical control is crossed, 251— Course of impulses leaving the cortex, 251— Size ot the pyramidal tracts in different mammals, 252. E. Localization in the Cerebral Cortex of the Cell-groups Receiving the Afferent Impulses • ~°2 Sensory regions of the cortex, 252— Delimitation of the sensory areas, 253— Hemi- anopsia 255— Association-fibres and association-centres (Flechsig), 256— Aphasia, 257 —Relative importance of the two hemispheres, 258— Composite character of incoming impulses, 260— Variations in association, 260 — Latent areas, 261. F. Comparative Physiology of the Divisions of the Encephalon 262 Methods of determining, 262— Removal of central hemispheres, 263— Functions of the corpus callosum, 270— Functions of the corpora striata, 271— Functions of the thalamus, 271— Functions of the cerebellum, 272. Part III.— Physiology of the Nervous System Taken as a Whole 274 A. Weight of the Brain and Spinal Cord 2/4 Weight of the encephalon and spinal cord, 274— Weight of the encephalon, 275 -In- terpretations of weight, 277— Weights of different portions of the encephalon, 277— Effect of social environment, 277— Brain-weight of criminals, 277— Brain-weights of different races, 278— Weight of the spinal cord, 278. B. Growth Changes 2<8 Growth of the brain, 278— Relation between growth of body and that of encephalon, 280— Increase in the number of functional nerve-elements, 280— Increase in the fibres of the cortex, 282— Significance of medullation, 283— Increase in the mass of the neu- rones, 283— Number of cells, 283— Change in specific gravity with age, 284. C. Organization and Nutrition of the Central Nervous System 285 ( >rganization in the central system, 285— Defective development of the central system, 285— The central nervous system of laboratory animals, 286— The blood-supply of the central system, 286— The influence of glands on the nervous system, 289 The influ- ence of starvation on the nervous system, 289— Fatigue of the central nervous system, 289— Daily rhythms in the activity of the central nervous system, 289— The time taken in central processes, 291. D. Sleep ■« Conditions favoring sleep, 291— Causes of sleep, 292— Condition of the central nervous system in sleep, 293 — Effect of loss of sleep, 295. E. Old Age of the Centbal System 295 Metabolism in the nerve-cells in old age, 295- -Decrease in weight of the brain in old age, 296 Changes in the encephalon in old age, 296— Changes in the cerebellum in old age, 296, THE SPECIAL SENSES -!,s A. Vision (By Hknky P. BOWDITCH) 298 The general physiology of vision. 298— The mechanical movements <>f the eyeballs around various axes, 298- The muscles of the eye. 299 The dioptric apparatus of the eye, 300 The refracting media of the eve. 302 The optical constants of the rye, 304 —The mechanism of accommodation, 306 The range of accommodation, 312 Myopia and hypermetropia, 313— Presbyopia, 314— Spherical aberration, 315 Chromatic aberration, 316 Astigmatism, :;i7 [ntraocular images, 320— Muscse volitantes, 320 —The retinal vessels, 32] circulation of blood in the retina, 322 The innervation and movements of the iris, 322 The principle of the ophthalmoscope, 326 The structure of the retina, 327 -The blind-spot of the retina. 328 Changes produced m the retina bv light, 330— The production of the sensation of light, 331 The qualita- tive modifications of light, 332 Color-sensations, 333 Means of producing color- 14 CO XT E NTS. PAGE mixtures, 333 — Color-theories, 335 — Color-blindness, 338 — The intensity of light sensations, 339 — The luminosity of different colors, 340 — The function of rods and cones, 341 — The saturation of color-sensations, 342 — Retinal stimulation, 343 — The latenl period <>f light-sensations, 343 -The rise to maximum of light-sensations, 3t3 — The fatigue of tin- retina, 344— The after-effect of stimulation, 315 — After-images, 346 — Color-contrasts, 346 -The perception of space. 347— Irradiation, 34!) — The false judgments of snbdivided Bpace, 350 — The perception of distance, 354 — Binocular vision, 356' — Pseudoscopic vision, 357— Binocular combination of colors, 358 — Corre- sponding points, 358- Visual illusions, 360. B. The Eab and Hearing (By Hknkv Skwall) 362 The anatomy and histology of the ear, 362 — The external ear, 362 — The middle ear, 36:; Movements of the ear-ossicles, 367 — The Eustachian tube, 369— The muscles of the middle ear, 369- The vibrations of the tympanic membrane, 370 — The structure of the internal ear, 371— The general anatomy of the cochlea, 374 — The transmission of vibrations through the labyrinth, 376 — The membranous cochlea and the organ of Corti, 376 — The theory of auditory sensations, 380. C. The Relations Between Physical and Physiological Sound 381 Production of sound-waves, 381 — Loudness and musical pitch, 381 — The tympanic membrane as an organ of pressure-sense, 382— Overtones and quality of sound, 3s3 — Analysis of composite tones by the ear. 384 — Inharmonic overtones, 386 — The produc- tion n( heats, 386 — Harmony and discord. 3S7 — Combinational tones, 387 — Auditory fatigue, 387 -Imperfections of the ear. 388 — Perceptions of time-intervals, 388 — Musi- cal tones and noises, 388 — Functions of different parts of the ear, 388 — The judgment of direction and distance, 389. D. CUTANEOU8 AND MUSCULAB SENSATIONS 390 General importance of the cutaneous and muscular sensations, 390 — Ending of sensory nerve-fibres in the skin, 391 — Relations of stimulus to the touch-sensations, 392 — The localization of touch-sensations. 394 — Pressure-points, 396 — The importance of the end-organ, 396— Touch-illusions. 396 — The temperature-sense, 397 — Cold and warm points. 398 — Common sensation and pain, 399 — Transferred or sympathetic pains; allochiria, 400 — Muscular sensation, 401 — Hunger and thirst, 404. E. The Equilibrium of the Body ; the Function of the Semicibculab Canals 404 The sense of equilibrium, 404 — Disturbances of the sense of equilibrium, 405 — Theory of the relation of the semicircular canals to equilibrium, 406 — The relation of the vestibular sacs to equilibrium, 407. F. Smell 408 Structure of the olfactorv epithelium, 408 — The production of olfactory sensations, 409. G. Taste 410 Structure of the taste-buds, 410 — The production of taste-sensations, 411 — Classifica- tion of taste-sensations, 412 — Specific energy of taste-nerves, 413. PHYSIOLOGY OF SPECIAL MUSCULAR MECHANISMS 414 A. The Action of Locomotor Mechanisms (By Warren P. Lombard) 414 The articulations, 414— Sutures, 414— Symphyses, 414— Syndesmoses, 414— Diar- throses, 115— The lever-action of muscles on bones, 417— The act of standing, 418 — The act of locomotion. 420— Walking, 420— Running, 421. B. Voice and Speech (By Henry Sewall) 421 Voice-production, 121 — Functions of the epiglottis, 422— Ventricular hands and ventricles of Morgagni, 122— The true vocal cords, 423— The cartilages of the larynx. 425— The muscles of the larynx. 125— Specific actions of the laryngeal muscles, 427— The nerve-supply of the larynx, 428 — The laryngoscopy appearance of the larynx, 429— The production of voice,' 130— Loudness and pitch of voice. 430— Quality of voice, 130 Arrangements for changing the pitch of the voice, 432— The vocal registers, 132 A whistling register, 433— Speech, 433— The classification of vowel sounds, 434— Whispering, 436 The production and classification of consonants, 436. REPRODUCTION (By Frederic S. Lee) 439 A. Bepboductiom in Genebal 439 Asexual reproduction, 439 — Sexual reproduction, 440— Origin of sex and theory of reproduction, 111 — Primary and secondary sexual characters, 442 — The sexual organs, 443. B. The Male Repboductive Organs : • 413 Structure and properties of the spermatozoon, I 13- Maturation of the spermatozoon, 445— The composition of semen, 445 -The testis, 146 Tin' urethra. 1 18— The prostate gland, II- -Cowper's glands, 448 — The penis, 41-". CONTENTS. 15 . PAGE C. The Female Eeproductive Organs 449 The ovum, 449 — Maturation of the ovum, 451 — The ovary and ovulation, 454 — The Fallopian tubes, 456 — The uterus, 456 — The act of menstruation, 157 — Comparative physiology of menstruation, 459 — Theories of menstruation, 460— -The vagina, 462 — The vulva and its parts, 462 — The mammary glands, 462 — The internal secretion of the ovaries, 462. D. The Reproductive Process 463 The act of copulation, 463 — Locomotion of the spermatozoa, 465 — Fertilization of the ovum, 466 — Segmentation of the ovum, 467 — Polyspermy, 471 — The decidua graviditatis, 471 — The fetal membranes, 472 — The placenta, 474 — Nutrition of the embryo, 475 — Physiological effects of pregnancy on the mother, 477 — The duration of gestation, 478 — Parturition, 479 — First stage of labor, 479 — Second stage of labor, 480 — Third stage of labor, 480 — Physiology of labor, 481 — Multiple conceptions, 482 — The determination of sex, 483. E. Epochs in the Physiological Life of the Individual 486 Growth of the cells, the tissues, and the organs, 486 — Growth of the body before birth, 486— Growth of the body after birth, 487— The condition of puberty, 489— The Climacteric, 490 — Senescence, 490 — Death, 491 — Theory of death, 492. F. Heredity 494 The facts of inheritance, 494 — Latent characters, atavism or reversion, 495 — Re- generation. 496 — The inheritance of acquired characters, 496 — The inheritance of dis- ease,498 — Theoriesof heredity, 498- < Jcmi-plasm, 499 — Variation, 500 — Darwin's theory of pangenesis, 501 — Weismann's theory of heredity, 502 — Theory of epigenesis, 504. I. GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. A. Introduction. It is seldom that the physical and chemical structure of a tissue, as revealed by the microscope aud the most careful analysis, gives even a suggestion as to its function. No one would conclude from looking at a piece of beef, or even microscopically examining a muscle, that it had once been capable of motion, nor would the most exact statement of its chemical composition give indication of such a form of activity. The most thorough histological and chemical examination of the bundle of fibres which compose a nerve would fail to sug- gest that a blow upon one end of it would cause to be transmitted to the other end an invisible change capable of exciting to action the cell with which the nerve communicated. To understand such a structure we must first learn the forms of activity of which the tissue is capable, the influences which excite it to action, and the conditions essential to its activity, and then seek an expla- nation of these facts in its physical and chemical constitution. Contractility. — One of the most striking properties of living matter is its power to move and to change its form. At times the movements occur apparently spontaneously, the exciting cause seeming to originate within the living substance, but more often the motions are developed in response to some external influence. This power finds its best expression in muscle-substance. In its resting form a muscle, such as the biceps, is elongated, and when it is excited to action it assumes a more spherical shape, i. e. shortens and thickens, whence it is said to have the property of contractility. It is the shortening, the contraction, of the muscle which enables it to perform its function of moving the parts to which it is attached, as the bones of the arm or leg, and of altering the size of the structures of which it forms a part, as the walls of the heart, intestine, or bladder. Ordinary muscle-substance is arranged in fine threads, each one of which is enveloped in a delicate membrane, the sarcolemma; these muscle-fibres can be compared to long sausages of micro- scopic proportions. A muscle is composed <>f m vasl Dumber of fibres arranged side by side in bundles, the whole being firmly bound together by connective tissue. Since isolated muscle-fibres have been seen under the microscope to contract, each fibre can be looked upon as containing true muscle- substance and being endowed with contractility. The movements of muscle- are the resultant of the combined activity of the many microscopic fibres of which the muscles are composed. The rate, extent, strength, and duration of muscular contractions are adapted Vol. II.— 2 17 18 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. to the needs of the parts to be influenced, and it is found that the structure of the muscles differs according to the work which they have to perform. Thus we find two large classes of muscles : the one, like the muscles which move the bones, remarkable for the rapidity with which they change their form, but unsuited to long-continued action ; the other, occurring in the walls of the intestine, blood-vessels, bladder, etc., sluggish of movement, but possessing great endurance. The first of these, when examined with the microscope, is seen to be composed of bundles of fibres, which are transversely marked by alter- nating dark and light bauds, and hence are called striated or striped muscles ; the other, though composed of fibres, shows no such cross markings, and therefore is known as smooth or non-striated muscle. Striated muscles are SL Fig. 1.— Amoeba proteus, magnified 200 times: a, endosarc; b, simple pseudopodium ; C, ectosarc; d, first stage in the growth of a pseudopodium : c, pseudopodium a little older than d ; /, branched pseudo- podium; g, food-vacuole ; h, food-ball; i, endoplast; k, contractile vesicle (after Brooks: Handbook of Invertebrate Zoology). often called voluntary, because most of them can be excited to action by the will, whereas non-striated muscles are termed involuntary, because in most cases they cannot be so controlled. Within these two large classes of muscles we find special forms presenting other, though lesser, differences in function and structure. The muscle of the heart, though striated, differs so much from other forms of striped muscle as almost to belong in a special class. GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. 19 Since contractility is possessed by all forms of muscle-tissue, it is evident that it is independent of superficial structural differences. Nor is muscle the only substance possessing this property. Even isolated microscopic particles of liv- ing matter are capable of making movements, both spontaneously and when excited by external influences. As far back as 1755, Rosel von Rosenhof described the apparently spontaneous changes in form of a living organism composed of a single cell, a fresh-water amoeba. Moreover, he noted that, if quiet, it could be excited to action by mechanical shocks. The amoeba (Fig. 1) is a little animal, of microscopic size, which is found in the ooze at the bottom of pools, or in the slime which clings to some of our fresh-water plants. Under the microscope it is seen to be composed of jelly- like, almost transparent matter, in which are a vast number of fine granules, a delicate tracery of finest fibrils, a small round body, called the nucleus or endoplast, a round hollow space termed the contractile vesicle, which is seen to change in size, appearing or disappearing from time to time, and small parti- cles, which are bits of food or foreign bodies. In the resting state the body has a somewhat flattened, irregular form, which, if the slide on which it rests be kept warm, is found to alter from minute to minute. Little tongue-like projec- tions, pseudopods (false feet), are protruded from the surface like feelers, and are then withdrawn, while others appear in new places. Evidently the little creature, though composed of a single cell, is endowed with life and has the power of making movements. Moreover, it may be seen to change its place, the method of locomotion being a peculiar one. One of the processes, or pseudopods, may be extended a considerable distance, and then, instead of being withdrawn, grow in size, while the body of the animal becomes corre- spondingly smaller ; thus a transfer of material takes place, and this continues until the whole of the material of the cell has flowed over to the new place. This power of movement per- mits the animal to eat. If when moving over the slide it encounters suitable food material, a diatom for instance, it flows round it, engulf- ing it in its semifluid mass; and in a similar manner the animal gets rid of the useless sub- stances which it may have surrounded, by flow- ing away from them. These movements may result from changes which have occurred within a,Ciiiaof ciliated disk; 6, ciliated disk; its own substance, and apparently independently ''• peristome; . oesophagus; n , • n r\ ii ,1 i i f, contractile vesicle; g, food-vacuoles ; of any external influence. On the other hand, ft.endoplast; Undosarc; Mctosarc; I, if its body be disturbed by being touched, by CQticle; m, axis of stem (after Brooks: • i • i Handbook of Invertebrate Zon!tid i., Th. 1, IS. 344. GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. 21 Irritability and conductivity are not confined to contractile mechan- isms. They are possessed in a still higher degree by nerve-cells, neurones, as they are called, which have not thus far been found to have the power of movement, except that which is associated with the growth of a cell. Each neurone is composed of a body and one or more branches. The bodies of the nerve-cells are located chiefly in the spinal cord and brain, a smaller number being found in the spinal ganglia and in the ganglia of the so-called sympa- thetic system. The branches of a neurone are of two kinds, an axis-cylinder process, or axone, which frequently carries at its extremity a specially formed organ, through which it is able to excite to action the cells with which it comes in contact, and protoplasmic processes, or dendrites, which have no such exciting mechanism, and are destined to receive excitation and transmit it to the body of the nerve-cell. Outside the central nervous system, at least, the axone and the dendrite acquire a delicate membranous sheath, the neurilemma, which invests it as the sarcolemma does the muscle-fibre. The branches of nerve-cells together with their sheaths form the nerve- fibres. There are two classes of nerve-fibres, medullated and non-medullated, which are distinguished by the fact that the former has between the axis- cyliuder and the neurilemma another covering composed of fatty material, called the medullary sheath, while in the latter this is absent. Just as it is the special function of the muscle-fibre to change its form when it is excited, so it is the special function of the nerve-fibre to transmit the condition of activity excited at one end throughout its length, and to awaken to action the cell with which it communicates. Nerve-fibres are the paths of communication between nerve-cells in the central nervous sys- tem, between sense-organs at the surface of the body and the nerve-cells, and between the nerve-cells and the muscle- and gland-cells. Nerve- fibres are distinguished as afferent and efferent, or centripetal and centrifugal, according as they carry impulses from the surface of the body inward or from the central nervous system outward. Further, they receive names according to the character of the activity which they excite : those which excite muscle- fibres to contract are called motor nerves; those distributed to the museles in the walls of blood-vessels, vaso-motor; those which stimulate gland-cells to action, secretory; those which influence certain nerve-cells in the brain and so cause sensations, sensory. Still other names are given, as "trophic" to fibres which are supposed to have a nutritive function, and " inhibitory " to those which check the activities of various organs. The method of conduction is the same in all these cases, the result depending wholly on the organ stimulated. Nerve-fibres do not run for any distance separately, but always in company with others. Thus large nerve-trunks may be formed, as in the case of the nerves to the limbs, in which afferent and efferent fibres run side by side, the whole being bound together into a compact bundle by connective tissue. The separate fibres, though thus grouped together, are anatomically and physiologi- cally as distinct as the wires of an ocean cable; that these many strands are bound together is of anatomical interest, but has little physiological significance. The active substance of the nerve-fibre does not show contractility, but this 22 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. does not prevent it from being classed with other irritable forms of living cell- substance as protoplasm. In spite of differences in structure and composition, nerve protoplasm and muscle protoplasm are found to have many points of resemblance. An explanation of the physiological resemblances may be found in their common ancestry. All the cells of the many structures of the animal body are descended from the two parent cells from which the animal isdeveloped. The fertilized ovum divides, and two cells are formed, these new cells divide, and so the process continues, the developing cells through unknown causes be- coming arranged to form more or less definite layers and groups, which by means of foldings and unequal growths develop into the various structures and organs of the fetus. At the same time that the division is going on, the total amount of material is increasing. Each of the cells absorbs and assimilates dead food- material, and this dead material is built into living substance. During this process of development and growth the cells of special tissues and organs acquire special anatomical and chemical characters. This development of specialized cells is termed cell-differentiation. Hand in hand with the ana- tomical and chemical differentiation goes a physiological differentiation. The protoplasm of each type of cell, while retaining the general characteristics of protoplasm, has certain physiological properties developed to a marked degree and other properties but little developed, or altogether lacking. The fertilized ovum does not have all the anatomical and chemical characteristics of all the cells which are descended from it, not at least in just the form in which they are possessed by these cells, and it cannot be assumed that its living sub- stance possesses all the physiological properties which are owned by its descendants. Many of these properties it must have, for many of them are essential to the continuance of life of all active cells, — such as the power to take in, alter, and utilize materials which are suitable for the building up and repair of the cell-substance, the power of chemically changing materials possessing potential energy so that the form of actual energy which is essential to the per- formance of the work of the cell shall be liberated, and the power to give off the waste materials which result from chemical changes. The protoplasm of the ovum, to have these powers, has properties closely allied to absorption, digestion, assimilation, respiration, excretion ; and, in consideration of the special function of the ovum, we may add that it possesses the property of reproduc- tion. The question of its possessing the characteristic properties of muscle and nerve protopla-m cannot be answered off-hand. Careful study, however, has shown the ovum of Hydra to possess irritability, conductivity, and contractility. It undergoes amoeboid movements, as was first shown by Kleinenberg. Balfour,1 in writing of the development of the ova of Tubularidae, which is of a type similar to Hydra, says: " The mode of nutrition of the ovum may be very instructively studied in this type. The process is one of actual feed- in-, much a- an amoeba might feed on other organisms." Something similar seems to be true of the ova of echinodermata. During impregnation various movements are described implying the properties of irritability, conductivity, and contractility. Thus in the case of Asterias glacialis, when the head of the 1 Comparative Embryology, pp. 17, 29. GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. 23 spermatozoon comes in contact with the mucilaginous covering of the ovum, "a prominence pointing toward the nearest spermatozoon now rises from the super- ficial layer of protoplasm of the egg and grows until it comes in contact with the nearest spermatozoon." " At the moment of contact between the sperma- tozoon and the egg, the outermost layer of protoplasm of the latter raises itself up as a distinct membrane, which separates from the egg and prevents the entrance of other spermatozoa." Some of the eggs of arthropods and other forms have likewise been observed to undergo amceboid movements as a result of the physiological stimulus given by the spermatozoon.1 Although irritability and contractility of the ovum have thus far been made out in but few forms, it is probable that they play an important part in all during fertilization and division. It would seem, then, that the ovum has all the principal properties which we ascribe to cell-protoplasm, and that these properties are inherited more or less completely developed by the many forms of cells descended from it. The protoplasm of specialized cells, in spite of their differences in structure, still retains its protoplasmic nature. Undoubtedly structural peculiarities are intimately related to specialized functions, — the striped muscle, for example, is especially adapted for rapid movements, and the nerve-fibre is remarkable for its power of conduction. Physiological methods for the examination of individual cells are as yet in their infancy, and we must still seek for exact knowledge of the functional activity of cells by observing the combined action of many cells of the same kind.2 B. Irritability of Muscle and Nerve. Irritability is the property of living protoplasm which causes it to undergo characteristic physical and chemical changes when it is subjected to certain influences, called irritants, or stimuli. By an irritant is meant an external influ- ence which, when applied to living protoplasm, as of a nerve or muscle, excites it to action. Irritants may be roughly classed as mechanical, chemical, thermal, and electrical. The normal physiological stimulus is developed within some of the nervous mechanisms of the body as the result of the activity of the nerve-protoplasm, this having been excited as a rule by some form of irritant. The degree of irritability of a given form of protoplasm is measured by the amount of activity which it displays in response to a definite irritant, or by the minimal amount of irritation required to excite it to action. If the irritant be applied directly to a muscle, the height to which the muscle contracts and raises a given weight may be taken as an indication of its activity. As the nerve gives no visible evidence of activity, the effecl of the irritant upoa it is usually estimated by the extent to which the organ stimulated by the nerve reacts ; in the case of motor nerves, the strength of the contraction of the corresponding muscle is taken as an index. To determine the exact relation of an irritant to its irritating effect we should 1 Korschelt: Zoologischea Jahrbueh, 1891, Aunt. Abtheil., Bd. iv., Heft 1, S. 1. Hertwig: Morpkologisches Jahrbuch, 1876, Bd. 1. Herbst: Biologische Centralblatt, L891, xiii. S. 22. 2 Kor the physiology of the lower forms of :niim:il life, see (,'cmr failure to act on the side which was exposed to the curare was because the end-plate had been paralyzed by the drug. By the use of curare, therefore, we are enabled to prevent the nerve-impulse from reaching the muscles, and. when we have done this, we find that the muscle is still able to respond to dired excitation with all forms of irritants, viz., 1 Ch. Bernard : " Analyse physiologiqne des Pmprietcs dee Systemes tnosculaires et nerveux au moveri du Curare," Otnnptes-rendus, 1856, p. 825. Kolliker: " Physiologische T'ntersuch- ungen iiber den Wirkunjjen einiger (lifte," Archiv fur pdthologische Anatomie, 1S56. GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. 27 electrical, mechanical, thermal, and chemical. Evidently the muscle-proto- plasm is irritable and is capable of developing1 a contraction independently of the nerves. There are a number of natural plant bases that have a " curare- like" action — c. g.} brucin, strychnin, leucin, nicotin, conin, etc.1 If a nerve-muscle preparation be dissected out and placed in a 0.7 per cent, solu- tion of sodium chloride containing one of these drugs, sooner or later the nerve- ends will be poisoned, and it will be found that excitation of the nerve has no effect on the muscle, although the muscle responds well to direct excitation. Other Proofs llutt the Muscle-protoplasm can be Directly Irritated. — Mus- cles with long parallel fibres, such as the sartorius of the frog, contain no nerves at their extremities, the nerve-fibres joining the muscle-fibres at some little distance from their ends. The tip of such a muscle, where no nerve-fibres can be discovered by the most careful microscopical exam- ination, is found to be irritable. The fact that in some of the lower animals there are simple forms of contrac- tile tissue in which nerves cannot be discovered, and which are irritable, is interesting as corroborative evi- dence, although it is not a proof, of the independent irritability of a highly differentiated tissue such as striated muscle. Another similar piece of evidence is to be found in the fact that the heart of the embryo beats rhythmically before nerve appears to have been developed. A proof can be found in the observation that if a nerve be cut it begins to undergo degenera- Fl«-4--curareexpenment: & ... the shaded parts shovi there tion and loses its irritability and conductivity in four gionofthebody to which the or five days, and the excitation of such a nerve has S^^fSS*™ no effect upon the muscle although direct stimulation protected by the ligature ,. ri i •, Mf • r> ii ii ,• » from the action of the drug. Of the muscle itself IS followed by contraction. As The unbroken lines represent degeneration involves not onlv the whole course of the the sensory nerves which ... " , . . carry sensorv impulses from nerve, but also the nerve end-plates, the contraction the skin to the central nerv- must be attributed to the irritability of the muscle- ous system ; the broken lines * m m indicate the motor nerves, substance. Another point of interest in this connection which carry motor impulses is the behavior of a dying muscle. If it be struck, &om the central nervous sys- * o 7 te]ll ,,,,( (I, (iR, muscles instead of contracting as a whole it contracts at the Lauder Brunton: Pharmacol i i •. • •■ ■ j ,i j • ,i t} otii/. Therapeutics, ami M place where it was irritated, the drawing together ot „,,/„.<,) the fibres at the part forming a local swelling, or welt. If such a muscle be stroked, a wave of contraction spreads over it, following the instrument, instead of extending, as under normal conditions, by mean.- of the excited nerve-fibres to other parts. Under these circumstances the circum- scribed contraction would seem to show that the nerves had lost their irrita- bility, or that the nerve-ends no longer transmitted the stimulus to the muscle, and the response was due to the direct excitation of the dying muscle-fibres. This phenomenon is known as an idiomuseiilar contraction. 1 Santesson : Archiv fur experimentelle Pathologic und Pharmakologie, 1 '.<•">, Bd. 35, S. 23. 28 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. Conditions which Determine the Effect of Excitation. The result of the irritation of nerve and muscle is dependent on two sets of conditions — namely, conditions which determine the irritability; condi- tions which determine the efficiency of the irritant. It will be necessary for us to study the second set of conditions first — for, before we can judge of the irritability and the effect of various influences upon it, we must consider how far the activity of the nerve and muscle is depend- ent on the character, strength, and method of application of the irritant. Conditions ■which Determine the Efficiency of Irritants. — Some of these conditions can be best studied on nerves, while others are more ap- parent in their effects on muscles. The most useful irritant for purposes of study is the electric current. Mechanical, thermal, and chemical irritants are likely to injure the tissue, and are not manageable, whereas electricity, if not too strong, can be applied again and again without producing any permanent alteration, and can be accurately graded as to strength, place, time, duration of application, etc. Of course, the results obtained by the use of a given irritant cannot be accepted for others until verified. The conditions which determine the effectiveness of the electric current as an irritant may be classed as follows : (a) The rate at which the intensity changes, (b) The strength of current, (c) The density of current. (<1) The duration of application, (e) The angle of application. (/) The direction of flow. Irritating Effect of the Electric < 'urrent. — Luigi Galvani, Professor of Physics at Bologna, 1791 (or, according to some, his wife Lucia), observed the legs of frogs which had been prepared for the kitchen, and had been suspended by brass hooks from an iron balcony, make convulsive movements every time the wind blew them against the iron. He repeated the experiment in his laboratory, and decided that the frogs had been excited to action by electric currents developed within themselves ; he looked upon the metals which he had used merely as conductors for this current. Volta, Professor of Natural Philosophy at Pavia, repeated Galvani's experiment, and concluded that there had been an electric current developed from the contact of the dissimilar metals with the moist tissues of the frog. In accordance with this idea he con- structed the voltaic pile, and this was the starting-point of the science of electricity of to-day. Although it is true that, under certain conditions, differences in electric potential sufficient to excite muscles to contraction can be developed in the animal body, the contractions of the frog's leg which Galvani observed were due to the metals which he employed. The experiment can be easily per- formed by connecting a bit of zinc to a piece of curved copper wire, and bring- ing the two ends of the arc against the moist nerve and muscle of a frog. A stronger and more efficient shock can be obtained from a Daniell or some other voltaic cell. A I>ll cell (Fig. 5) is composed of a zinc and copper plate, the former dipping into dilute sulphuric acid, the latter into a strong copper-sulphate solution. Although gravity will keep these liquids separated, if the cell is to be moved about it is better GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. 29 to enclose one of tlieni in a porous cup. A common form of cell consists of a glass jar, in the middle of which is a porous cup; outside the cup is the sulphuric acid and tie zinc plate, and inside the cup is the copper sulphate solution and the copper plate. The zinc plate is acted upon by the sulphuric acid, and, as a result of the chemical change, a difference of electric potential is set up between the metals, so that if the zinc and copper be connected by a piece of metal, what we call an electric current flows from the zinc to the copper inside the cell, and from the copper to the zinc outside the cell. The zinc plate, being the seat of the chemical change, is called the positive plate, and the copper the negative plate. Several such cells may be connected together to form a battery, each cell adding to the electro-motive force, and hence to the strength of the current. As the current is always considered to flow from + to — , we call the end of the wire connected with the copper (negative plate) the positive pole, or anode, and the end of the wire connected with the zinc (positive plate) the negative pole, or kathode. If one of these wires be touched to a nerve, under ordinary circumstances no effect is produced ; but when the other wire is likewise brought in contact with the nerve, the moist tissues of the nerve form a conductor, complete the cir- cuit, and an electric current at once flows through the nerve from the anode to the kathode. The effect of the sudden flow of electricity into the nerve is to give it a shock — as we say, it irritates the nerve — and the muscle which the nerve controls is seen to contract. In the place of using ordinary wires for applying the electricity, we use electrodes. These are practically the same thing, but have insulated handles, and have a form better suited to stimulate nerves or other tissues. The two wires may be held in two different Fig. 5.— Daniell cell. ^r X) : j V ^> Fig. 6.— a, Ordinary electrode for exciting exposed nerves and muscles, consisting of two wires enclosed, except at their extremities, in a handle of non-conducting material : b, C, non-polarizable elec- trodes. When metals come in contact with moist tissues a galvanic action is likely to occur and polariz- ing currents to be formed. These extra currents would complicate or Interfere with the results of many forms of experiment, and they are avoided by the use of non-polarizable electrodes. A simple form con- sists of a short glass tube, at one end of which is a plug of china day mixed with a 0.6 percent, solution of sodium chloride, and a1 the other end a cork through which an amalgamated zinc rod is thrust. The zinc rod dips into a saturated solution of zinc sulphate, which is in contact with the clay. The clay plugs touch the tissue to be excited, and the current passes from the zinc rods through the zinc-sulphate and sodium-chloride solutions in the (day to the tissues; d /, electrodes for exciting human nerves and mus- cles through the skin (after Erb): these may be of various forms and sizes, and arc arranged to screw into handles {g), to which the wires are attached; they are usually made of brass and covered with sponge or other absorbent material wet with salt -solution. The smaller electrodes are used when a dense, well-localized stream is required, and the larger electrodes when little action Is Wished and it is of advantage to have the stream diffuse. handles, in which case we speak of the positive and negative electrodes, or the anode and the kathode, or they may be held in the same handle (Fig. 6). 30 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. Keys— It is not as convenient to stimulate a nerve by touching it with the electrodes as it is to place it upon the electrodes and close the connection between the zinc and copper at some other part of the circuit ; this may be done by what is called a key. Any mechanism which can be used to complete the circuit could receive this name, and there are a number of convenient forms. The one most used by physiologists is that devised by Du Bois-lley- niond, and which bears his name (see Fig. 7). This has the advantage of being capable of being used in two different ways — one simply as a means to close the circuit, and the other to short-circuit the current. These two meth- ods are shown in Figure S. By the former method the key supplies a movable piece of metal by which contact be- tween the two ends of the wires may be made as in a (Fig. 8), or broken as in b, and the current be sent through the nerve, or prevented from entering it. By the latter method the battery is all the time connected with the electrodes, and the key acts as a movable bridge between the wires, and when closed gives a path of slight resistance by which the current can return to the battery without passim; through the nerve. The current always takes the path of least resistance, and so, if the key be closed as in c, all the cur- rent will pass through the key and none will go to the nerve, which has a high resistance, whereas if the key be opened as in d, the bridge being removed, all the current will go through the nerve. It is often better to let the cell or battery work a short time and to get its full strength before letting the current enter the nerve, and the short-circuiting key permits of this. Moreover, there are times when a nerve may be stimulated if connected Fig. 7.— Electric key. Fig. 8.— Electric circuiting. with the source of electricity by only one wire ; when the nerve is so excited, it is called unipolar stimulation; this may be prevented by the short-circuiting key. As has been said, a nerve is irritated if it be connected with a battery and an electric current suddenly passes through it. Unless the current be very strong the irritation is transient, however ; the muscle connected with the GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. 31 nerve gives a single twitch at the moment that the current enters the nerve, and then remains quiet; and thus we meet with the remarkable fact that an electric current, though irritating a nerve at the moment that it enters it, can flow through the nerve continuously without exciting it. Fur- ther, although the current while flowing through the nerve does not excite it, a sudden withdrawal of the current from the nerve irritates it, and causes the muscle connected with it to contract. It is our custom to speak of closing, or making, the circuit when we complete the circuit and let the current flow through the nerve, and of opening, or breaking, the circuit when we withdraw the current from the nerve. Since the closing of the circuit acts as a sudden irritant to the nerve, we speak of this irritant as a "making" or "closing" shock, and the corresponding contraction of the muscles as a making or closing contraction ; similarly we speak of the effect of opening the circuit as an "opening" or " breaking" shock, and the result- ing contraction as an opening or breaking contraction. As we shall see later, the making contraction excited by the direct battery current is stronger than the breaking contraction : the explanation of this must be deferred (see page 38). (a) Effect of the Rate at which an Irritant is Applied, Illustrated by the Elec- tric Current. — As has been said, an electric current of constant medium strength Fig. 9.— Rheonome. does not irritate a nerve while flowing through it, but the nerve is irritated at the instant that the current enters it, and at the instant that the current leaves it. Is it the change of condition to which the nerve is subjected, or is it the suddenness of the change, which produces the excitation? Would it be possi- ble to turn an electric current into a nerve and remove it from a nerve so slowly that it would not act as an irritant? The experiment has been tried, and it has been found that if the nerve be subjected to an electric current ihc strength of which is increased or decreased very gradually, no change occurs in the oerve sufficient to cause a contraction of the muscle. In this experiment, instead of using the ordinary key, we close and open the circuit by means of a rheonome (see Fig. 9). This instrument contains a fluid resistance, which can be altered at will, thereby per- mitting a greater or less strength of current to pass from the battery into tin: circuit 32 AN AMERICAN TEXT- BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. containing the nerve. The wires from the battery are connected with binding-posts, a, 6 (Fig. 9), at opposite sides of a circular groove containing a saturated solution of zinc sul- phate. Strips of amalgamated zinc connect the binding-posts with the fluid, and so com- plete a circuit which offers much resistance to the passage of the current. From the centre of the block containing the groove rises an upright bearing a movable horizontal bar. from each extremity of which an amalgamated zinc rod. e and/, descends and dips into the zinc- sulphate solution. The zinc roils are connected with binding-posts on the movable bar, and from these wins pass to the electrodes on which the nerve rests. The bar revolves on a pivot on the top of the upright, and thus the zinc rods can be readily approached to or removed from the zinc strips, the poles of the battery. When the zinc rods hold a position midway between these poles, the current all passes by the way of the fluid. As the bar is turned, so as to bring the zinc rods nearer and nearer the two poles of the bat- tery, the current divides, and more and more of it passes through the path of lessening resistance of which the nerve is a part. When the zinc rods are brought directly opposite the poles of the battery nearly all the current passes by the way of the nerve. If the bar be turned more or less rapidly, the current is thrown into, or withdrawn from, the nerve more or less quickly. By this arrangement we can not < ml v observe that the nerve fails to be irritated when the current is made to enter or leave it gradually, and when it is flowing continuously through it, but that sudden variations in the density of the cur- rent flowing through the nerve, such as are caused by quick movements of the bar, although they do not make or break the circuit, serve to excite. This experiment shows that electricity, as such, does not irritate a nerve, but that a sudden change in the density of the current, whether it be an increase or decrease, produces an alteration in the nerve-protoplasm which excites it to action and causes the development of what we call the nerve-impulse. Du Bois-Reymond's Law. — Du Bois-Revmond formulated the following rule for the irritation of nerves by the electrical current : " It is not the abso- lute value of the current at each instant to which the motor nerve replies by a contraction of its muscle, but the alteration of this value from one moment to another; and, indeed, the excitation to movement which results from this change is greater the more rapidly it occurs by equal amounts, or the greater it is in a given time" We shall have occasion to see that this rule has exceptions, or rather that there is an upper as well as lower limit to the rate of change of density of the electric current which is favorable to irritation. Similar observations may be made with other forms of irritants. Pres- sure, if brought to bear on a nerve gradually enough, may be increased to the point of crushing it without causing sufficient irritation to excite the attached muscle to contract, although, as has been said, a very slight tap is capable of stimulating a nerve. Temperature, and various chemicals, likewise, must be so applied as to produce rapid alterations in the nerve-protoplasm in order to act as irritant-. The same rule would seem to hold good for the nerve-cells of the central nervous system. It is a matter of daily experience that the nervous mechanisms through which sensory impressions are perceived are vigorously excited by sudden alterations in the intensity of stimuli reaching them, and but little affected by their continuous application; the withdrawal of light, a sudden GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. 33 alteration of temperature, an unexpected noise, or the cessation of a monotonous sound, as exemplified by the common experience that a sleeper is awakened Fig. 10.— Induction apparatus : a, primary coil ; b, secondary coil ; c, the automatic interrupter. when reading aloud abruptly ceases, attract the attention, although a continu- ous sensory irritation may be unnoticed. This physiological law of the nervous system would seem to have a psychological bearing as well. Fig. 11.— Schema of induction apparatus. Irritating Effect of Induced Electric Currents. — Within certain limits, the more rapid the change in intensity of an electric current the greater its power to irritate. This probably accounts in part for the fact that the induced current is a more powerful irritant to nerves than the direct galvanic current. Induced currents are usually obtained by means of an induc- tion apparatus (see Fig. 10). The ordinary induction apparatus employed in the laboratory (see Fig. 11) consists of a coil of wire, p, which may be connected with the ter- minals of a battery, b, and a second coil, s, wholly independent of the first, which is connected with electrodes, e. At the instant that the key, h, in the primary circuit is closed, and the battery cur- rent enters the primary coil, an induced current s developed in the secondary coil, and the nerve resting on the electrodes is irritated. The in- duced current is of exceedingly short duration, suddenly rising to full intensity and falling to of fail of the Intensity of the primary cur- zero. As long as the battery current continues to rcnt when u is broken; 4, curve of tin- rise a ,i ., i .1 • -l n • and lull of intensity of the corresponding in- flow constant ly tliroimli ( lie pnniarv coil, ( here is , , . ■ ' ' _ _• duced current. no change in the electrical condition of the sec- ondary coil, but at the instant (lie primary current is broken another induced current of short duration is set up in the secondary coil, and again the nerve receives a shock. The rise and V,,,.. ti._ 3 ^ m*** — ^^>-^ 4 Fig. 12.— Schema of the relative intensity of induction currents (after Hermann, Hand buck dir Physioloffie, Bd. ii. 8. 87): /', abscissa for the primary current ; S, abscissa for the secondary current; 1, curve of the rise of intensity of the primary current when made; 2, curve of the rise and fall of intensity of 34 AN AMKIUCAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. fall of the density of the current in the secondary coil is very rapid, and this rapid double change in density of the current causes the induction shock to be a very effective irritant. The break- ing induction shock, as we call that which is produced by breaking the primary current, is found to act more vigorously than the making shock, which is the reverse of what is found with direct battery currents. The cause of this lies in the nature of the apparatus. At the moment that the current begins to flow into the primary coil, it induces not only a current in the secondary coil, but also currents in the coils of wire of the primary coil. These extra induced currents in the primary coil have the opposite direction to the battery cur- rent and tend to oppose its entrance, and thereby to prevent it from immediately gain- ing its full intensity. This delay affects the development of the induced current in the secondary coil, causing it to be weaker and to have a slower rise and fall of intensity than would otherwise be the case. When the primary current is broken, on the other hand, there is no opposition to its cessation, and the current induced in the secondary coil is intense and has a rapid rise and fall. These differences are illustrated in Figure 12. Myogram. — To accurately test the effect of the making and breaking induction shocks, it is necessary to record the reaction of the nerve; this can be done by recording the extent to which the corresponding muscle contracts in response to the stimulus which it receives from the nerve. In such an experiment it is customary to use what is known as a nerve-muscle prepara- tion. The gastrocnemius muscle and sciatic nerve of a frog, for instance, are carefully dissected out, the attachment of the muscle to the femur being pre- served, and the bone being cut through at such a point that a sufficiently long piece of it shall be left to fasten in a clamp, and so support the muscle (see Fig. 13). Fig. 13.— Method of recording muscular contraction. The simplest method of recording the extent of the muscular contraction is to connect the muscle by means of a fine thread with a light lever, and let the point of the lever rest against a smooth surface covered with soot, so that when the muscle contracts it shall draw up the lever and trace a line of cor- responding length upon the blackened surface. The combination of instru- GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. 35 ments employed to record the contraction of a muscle is called a myograph, and the record of the contraction is termed a myogram. If, when the muscle of a nerve-muscle preparation is thus arranged to write its contractions, the nerve be irritated with alternating mak- ing and breaking induction shocks of medium strength, the muscle will make a series of movements, which, if the surface be moved past the writing-point a short distance after each contraction, will be pictured in the record as a row of alternating long and short lines, the piG 14 _Effect of making records of the breaking contractions being higher than and breaking induction those of the making contractions (Fig. 14). Similar results are obtained if, instead of irritating the nerve, we irritate the curarized muscle directly. Stimulating Effects of Making and Breaking the Direct Battery Current. — On account of the construction of the induction apparatus, breaking induction shocks are more effective stimuli than making induction shocks. The reverse is true of the stimulating effects which come from making and breaking the direct battery current. The excitation which results from sending a galvanic current into a nerve or muscle is stronger than that which is caused by the withdrawal of the current. This difference is due to the physiological altera- tions produced by the current as it flows through the irritable substance, and is without doubt closely associated with changes in the irritability which occur at the moment of the entrance and exit of the current. The making contraction starts from the kathode, and the breaking contraction from the anode. The irritation process which results from making the current is developed at the kathode, and that which results from breaking the current is developed at the anode. This was first demonstrated on normal muscles by Von Bezold,1 and has since been substantiated for nerves as well as muscles Fig. 15.— Schema of Ilering's double myograph: C, clamp holding middle "I' muscle ; P.P. pulleys to the axes of which the recording levers are attached ; p,p, pulleys for the light weights w hich keep the muscle under slight tension; A, positive electrode; K, negative electrode ; r, commutator for reversing the current ; k, key ; b, battery. by the experiments of a great many observers. Perhaps the most striking demonstration is to be obtained by Engelmann's method. The positive and negative electrodes are applied to the two extremities of a long curarized sarto- 1TJntcrxiiclnui 10 SO SO 4" 100 11" '0> b 10, / 0 10 20 20' 10 0 d \i e f / \ Fio. 24. .—Schema of relation of the method of application of the electric current to the irritating effect. tion, illustrated by/, though not affecting nerves, might suffice for striated muscles and be favorable to the excitation of non-striated muscles. In the case of nerves, duration of current is less important than a rapid change of intensity. In the case of striated muscles the advantage to be gained by rapid variations can be easily overstepped, and the importance of the duration of the current is greater; while in the case of non-striated muscles duration of current is of the first importance and rapid variation may tail to excite. In the case of all tissues, strength and density of current, what we may call intensity of current, is favorable to excitation. (/) Effect of tin1 Direction in which the ( \wreni flows along the Neme. — The result of the irritating change produced in a nerve by a battery current has been found to depend upon whether the current Hows toward or away from the organ stimulated by the nerve. This fact can be most readily ob- served in the ease of isolated motor nerves. In the case of these nerves, the effects produced by opening and closing the current are different according as the current is descending, i. e. flows through the nerve in the direction of the muscle, or ascending, i. e. flows through the nerve in the opposite direction. Vol. II.— I 50 AN AMERICAN TEXT- BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. Moreover, by a given rate of change of intensity, the stimulating effect varies with the Btrength of the current employed. Pfliiger in his celebrated mono- graphj Untermchungen uber die Physiologie des Eleldrotonus, published in Berlin, 1859, p. 454, formulated the following rule for the result of excitations under varying conditions: Pfliiger's Law of Contraction. Ascending Current. Closing. Opening. Weak current Contr. Eest. Medium " Contr. Contr. Strong " Eest. Contr. Descending Current. Closing. Opening. Contr. Rest. Contr. Contr. Contr. Rest. To understand this so-called " law of contraction " we must bear in mind certain fundamental facts, namely: a. When a nerve is subjected to a battery current, an excitatory process is developed in the part of the nerve near the kathode when the current is closed, and in the part of the nerve near the anode when the current is opened (see p. 38). b. The excitatory process developed at the kathode is stronger than that developed at the anode (see p. 38). c. A third fact which is of no less importance, and which will be considered in detail when we study the effects of the constant current on the irritability and conductivity of nerve and muscle (see p. 95), is the following: During the time that a strong constant current is flowing through a nerve, the conduct- ing power is somewhat lessened in the part to which the kathode is applied, and is greatly decreased, or altogether lost, in the region of the anode ; moreover, at the instant that the current is withdrawn from the nerve the conducting power is suddenly restored in the region of the anode, and greatly lessened, or lost, in the region of the kathode. Ascending Current. K A Descending Current. A K Weak current Strong current Fig. 25.— Diagram illustrating Pfliiger's law. The twelve cases i Deluded in the above table can be represented in the fol- lowing diagram ( Fig. 25), in which a cross is marked at the part of the nerve GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. 51 from which the irritation that is effective in producing a contraction takes its rise. In the case of fresh motor nerves of the frog, when the current is weak, only closing excitations, i. c, those originating at the kathode, are effective by both directions of the current. As the strength of the current is increased, at the same time that the closing kathodic contractions grow stronger, opening anodic contractions begin to appear; and with currents of medium strength both closing and opening contractions are obtained with both directions of the current. If the strength of the current be still further increased, a change is observed ; with a strong current, the closing of the ascending and the opening of the descending current tails to excite a muscular contraction. This fact is demon- strated most clearly if we employ two nerve-muscle preparations, and lay the nerves in opposite directions across the non-polar- izable electrodes, so that the current from the battery shall flow through one of the nerves in an ascending direction and through the other in the descending direction (see Fig. 26). If under these conditions a strong battery current be employed, muscle a (through the nerve of which the current is descending) will contract only when the circuit is closed, and muscle b (through the nerve of which the current is ascending) will con- tract only when the circuit is opened. Since in the case of currents of medium strength, both opening and clos- ing the circuit, when the current is ascending and when it is descending, develops a condition of excitation in the nerve sufficient to cause contractions, the failure of the contraction by the closing of the strong ascending current, and by the opening of the strong descending current, can scarcely be supposed to be due to a failure of the exciting process to be developed in the nerve ; and it would seem more likely that the nerve-impulse is tor some reason prevented from reaching the muscle — which, as has been said, is the fact, the region of the anode being incapable of conducting during the flow of a strong current, and the region of the kathode losing its power to conduct at the instant such a current is opened. Effect of Battery Currents upon Normal Human Nerves. — In experi- ments upon normal human nerves, the current cannot be applied directly to the nerve, but has to be applied to the skin over the nerve. As it passes from the anode, the positive electrode, through the skin, the threads of current spread through the fluids and tissues beneath, somewhat as the bristles of a brush spread out, and the current flows in a more or less diffuse stream toward the point of exit, where the threads of current concentrate again to enter the kathode, the negative electrode. This spread of the current is illustrated in Figure 27. \? — \r A >—* K Fig. 26.— Effect of direction of current as shown by simultaneous excitation of two nerve-muscle preparations. 52 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. Fig. 27.— Rough schema of active threads of current by the ordinary application of electrodes to the skin over a nerve (ulnar nerve in the upper arm). The inactive threads arc given in dotted lines (after Erb: Ziemsseu's Pathologie und Therapie, Bd. iii. S. 76). The density of the current entering any structure beneath the skin will depend in part upon the size of the electrode directly over it — that is, the amount to which the current is concentrated at its point of en- trance or exit — in part on the nearness of the structure to the skin, and in part on the con- ductivity of the tissues of the organ in question as compared with the tissues and fluids about it. If the conditions be such as are given in Figure 27, the current will not, as in the case of the isolated nerve, enter the nerve at a given point, flow longitudinally through it, and then leave it at a given point ; most of the threads of current will pass at varying angles di- agonally through the part of the nerve beneath the positive pole, then flow through the fluids and tissues about the nerve, until, at a point beneath the negative pole, the concentrating threads of current again pass through the nerve. A distinction is to be drawn between the physical and physiological anode and kathode. The physical anode is the extremity of the positive electrode, and the physical kathode is the extremity of the negative electrode ; the physiological anode is the point at which the current enters the tissue under consideration, and the physiological kathode is the point where it leaves it. There is a physiological anode at every point where the current inters the nerve, and a physiological kathode at every point where it leaves the nerve; therefore there is a physiological anode and kathode, or groups of anodes and kathodes, lor the part of the nerve beneath the positive electrode, and another physiological anode and kathode, or collection of anodes and kathodes, for the part of the nerve beneath the negative electrode. To understand the effect upon the normal human nerve of opening and closing the battery current, it is necessary to bear in mind three facts, viz. : 1. At the moment that a battery current is closed, an irritating process is developed at the physiological kathode, and when it is opened, at the physio- logical anode. 2. The irritating process developed at the kathode on the closing of the current is stronger than that developed at the anode on the opening of the current. 3. The effect of the current is greatest where its density is greatest. The amount of the irritation process developed in a motor nerve is esti- mated from the amount of the contraction of the muscle. The contraction GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. 53 which results from closing the current, the closing contraction as it is called, represents the irritating change which occurs at the physiological kathode, while the contraction which results from opening the current, the opening contrac- tion, represents the irritating change developed at the physiological anode. Since there are physiological anodes and kathodes under each of the two elec- trodes— the physical anode and physical kathode (see Fig. 28) — four possible cases may arise, namely : 1. Anodic closing contraction — i. e. the effect of the change developed at Fig. 28.— Diagram showing physical and physiological anodes and kathodes: A, the physical anode, or positive electrode ; K, the physical kathode, or negative electrode ; a, a, o, physiological anodes ; k, k, k, physiological kathodes. the physiological kathode, the place where the current leaves the nerve, beneath the physical anode (the positive pole). 2. Anodic opening contraction — i.e., the effect of the change developed at the physiological anode, where the current enters the nerve, beneath the physical anode (the positive pole). 3. Kathodic closing contraction. — i. c. the effect of the change developed at the physiological kathode, where the current leaves the nerve, beneath the physical kathode (the negative pole). 4. Kathodic opening contraction — i. e., the effect of the change developed at the physiological anode, where the current enters the nerve, beneath the physical kathode (the negative pole). For convenience these four cases are represented by the abbreviations ACC, AOC, KCC, and KOC. Since the irritation process developed at a physiological kathode In- closing a current, is, other things being equal, stronger than that developed at a physiological anode by opening the current, we should expect that the two closing contractions, KCC and ACC, would be stronger than the two opening contractions, KOC and AOC. This is the case, and as the current is more dense in the region of the physiological kathode, beneath the physical kathode, than at the physiological kathode, beneath the physical anode, KCC is stronger than ACC. Of the two opening contractions, AOC is stronger than KOC because of the greater density of the current in the region of the physiological anode, 54 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. beneath the physical anode, than in the region of the physiological anode, beneath the physical kathode. These differences in the strength of the irritation process developed in these different regions is well shown by examining the reaction of nerves to cur- rents of gradually increasing strength. The effect of the opening and closing irritation is seen to be as follows : Weak currents. Medium currents. Strong currents. K< i KCC KCC ACC ACC AOC AOC KOC The natural order, therefore, would be KCC, ACC, AOC, KOC. Some- time-, however, AOC is stronger than ACC; this happens when on account of the relation of the surrounding tissues to the nerve the density of the cur- rent at the physiological anode is great as compared with the density at the physiological kathode. Bordier1 tested the strength of battery current neces- sary to awaken minimal sensations by unipolar excitations, and found the effect to be greatest by KC, then AC, then At ) ; and that it was least by Kl) — i.e., sensory behave like motor nerves. In testing the effect of the battery current on the nerves and muscles of man, it is customary to use one small and one large electrode (Fig. 6, d, <',/). The small electrode is placed over the part to be stimulated, while the large electrode is put over some distant portion of the body. This arrangement causes the current to be condensed, and hence efficient, when it enters or leaves the small exciting electrode, and to be diffused, and hence ineffective, at the large indifferent electrode. For example, the indifferent electrode may be placed on the sternum or over the back of the neck, while the excit- ing electrode may be put over the ulnar nerve at the elbow. The two poles may be connected with the battery, a pole-changer, rheostat, milliamperemeter, and exciting-key being introduced in the circuit. The pole-changer permits the exciting pole to be made A or K at the wish of the operator, the rheostat allows the strength of current to be raised gradually, and the milliampere- meter shows the strength of the current employed. With this arrangement the renetion of the nerve can be readily tested. When the currents employed are strong, it occasionally happens in the case of meu thai not only are the make and break followed by the usual rapid contractions of short duration, but during the closure of the current there is a continued contraction — galvanotonous, as it is sometimes called. This is especially seen under certain pathological conditions. When the nerve or muscle is diseased we may have the above order changed, and A( !< ! obtained with weaker currents than KOC, and KOC than AOC (Babinski) 2. This is known as the reaction of degeneration. Under 1 Bordier: Archives de Physiologie normaleet Pathohgique, 1897, ]>i> 543-553. 2 I5;ii>in.ski : Comples rendus de la Sociele de Bioloyie, 1899, p. o4o. GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. 55 such circumstances the nerve might respond well to the direct battery current and yet fail to respond to the induced current. This would be still more markedly the case with the muscle, which at the same time that it gave no response to induction shocks would react better than normally to battery currents. At such times galvanotonus is easily excited. Thus during degeneration the irritability of the nerve and muscle approaches that of slowly reacting forms of protoplasm (see p. 70). Conditions -which Determine the Irritability of Nerves and Muscles. — We have thus far considered the conditions which determine the efficiency of such an irritant as the electric current. Other irritants are subject to like conditions, their activity being controlled to a considerable extent by the sud- denness, strength, density, duration, and, possibly, direction of application. It is not necessary for us to consider how each special form of irritant is affected by these conditions; it will be more instructive for us to study how different irritants alter the irritability of nerve and muscle, and the relation of irri- tability to the state of excitation. The power to irritate is intimately connected with the power to heighten irritability — for a condition of heightened irritability is difficult to distin- guish from a state of excitation. The irritability of cell-protoplasm is very dependent upon its physical and chemical constitution, and even slight altera- tions of this constitution, such as may be induced by various irritants, will modify the finely adjusted molecular structure upon which the normal response to irritants depends. If this change be in the direction of increased irritability, the result may be irritation. But we must defer the discussion of the relation of irritability to irritation until we have considered the conditions upon which the irritability of nerve and muscle depends. These conditions can be best studied in connection with the influences which modify them — namely : (a) Irritants. (6) Influences which favor the maintenance of the normal physiological condition. (c) The effects of functional activity. («) The Influence of Irritant* upon the Irritability of Nerve and Muscle. — Effect of Mechanical Agencies. — A sudden blow, pinch, twitch, or cut excites a nerve or muscle. All have experienced the effect of a mechanical stimulation of a sensory nerve, through accidental blows on the ulnar nerve where it passes over the elbow, " the crazy bone." The amount of mechanical energy required to cause a maximal excitation of an exposed motor nerve of a frog is estimated by Tigerstedt1 to be 7000 to 8000 milligrammillimeters, which would corre- spond roughly to a weight of 0.500 gram filling fifteen millimeters — at least a hundred times less energy than that given out by the muscles in response to the nerve-impulse developed. Such stimuli can be repeated a great many times, if not given at too shori intervals, without interfering with the activity 1 Studien iiber niechanisclie Nervenreizung," Acta Societatis Scientiarum Fennica, 1880, Bd. xi. S. 82. 56 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. of the nerve. A nerve can be irritated thirty to forty times, at intervals of three to four minutes, by blows from a weight of 0.485 gram, falling 1 to 20 millimeters, the contractions of the muscle, weighted with 30 to 50 grams, varying from minimal (<> from •'> to 4 millimeters in height. Rapidly following light blows or twitches applied to a motor nerve, by the tetanomotorof Heiden- hain or Tigerstedt, excite a -cries of contractions in the corresponding muscles which fuse more or less into a form of continuous contraction, known as tetanus. Not only may a nerve be excited by bringing sudden pressure to bear on it, but the sudden removal of weights or a sudden lessening of tension irritates.1 Kiihne lone a called attention to the excitation of sensory fibres of the ulnar nerve of man on the removal of pressure. The cause is probably the irregular return of the semi-fluid parts of the nerve to their normal relations. Mechanical applications to nerve and muscle first increase and later lessen and destroy the irritability. Thus pressure gradually applied first increases and later reduces the power to respond to irritants. Stretching a nerve acts in a similar way, for this also is a form of pressure; as Valentin said, the stretch- ing causes the outer sheath of the nerve to compress the myelin, and this in turn to compress the axis-cylinder. Tigerstedt states:2 "From a tension of 0 up to 20 grams the irritability of the nerve is continually increased, but it lessens as soon as the weight is further increased." Surgicallv the stretching of nerves is sometimes employed to destroy their excitability. Slight stretching heightens the excitability and even quite vigor- ous stretching has only a temporary depressing effect unless it be carried to the point of doing positive injury to the axis-cylinder, and of causing degen- eration. As nerves have the power to regenerate, they may recover from even such an injury. The irritability of muscles is likewise increased by moderate stretching and destroyed if it be excessive. Thus slight stretching produced by a weight causes a muscle to respond more vigorously to irritants. Similarly tension of the muscles of the leg, produced by slight over-flexion or extension, makes them more irritable to reflex stimuli, as in the case of the knee-jerk and ankle- clonus. Tension must be very marked to permanently alter the irritability of the muscles. Effect of Temperature. — Changes in temperature, if sudden and extreme, irritate nerves and muscles. If the nerve or muscle be quickly frozen or plunged into a hot fluid it will be excited and the muscle be seen to contract. The cause of the irritation has been attributed to mechanical or chemical alterations produced by the change of temperature. The ulnar nerve at the elbow is excited if the part be dipped into ice-water and allowed to remain there until the cold has had time to penetrate; as is proved by the fact that in addition to the sensations from the skin, pain is felt which is attributed by the subject of the experiment to the region supplied by the nerve. As the effect 1 v. I'xhull: Zetockrifl, fur Biologic, 1894, Bd. xxxi. S. 118: 1895, Bd. xxxii. S. 438. 2 Op. cit., S. 43. GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. 57 of the cold becomes greater the pain is replaced by numbness, both the irrita- bility and power of conduction of the nerve being reduced. Gradual cooling of motor nerves or muscles, and gradual heating, even to the point of death of the tissue, fails to excite contractions. It is stated that if a frog whose brain has been destroyed is placed in a bath the temperature of which is very gradually increased, the heating may be carried so far as to boil the frog without active movements having been called out. If a muscle be heated to 45° C. for frogs and 50° C. for mammals, it undergoes a chemical change, which is accompanied by a form of shortening different from the contraction induced by irritants. This form of contraction, though extensive, is feeble and is asso- ciated with a stiffening of the muscle, known as rigor ccdoris (see p. 164). In general it may be said that raising the temperature above the usual tem- perature of the animal increases, while cooling decreases, the irritability of the nerves and muscles. This statement requires to be amplified, because the character of the stimulus has a marked effect upon the result. Cooling the nerve increases its irritability for mechanical and chemical stimuli, for the constant current if it lasts at least 0.005 sec, for condenser discharges, and for sine currents of at least 0.005-0.01 sec. duration : heating; the nerve increases its irritability for these forms of electrical excitation when of shorter duration, and also for induced currents.1 If a nerve be excited by charging or discharging a condenser through it, the size of the condenser plays an important part, because it determines the duration of the stimulus ; for example a slow, prolonged rate of discharge may excite a nerve at 4° C. and fail to excite one at 30° C, while a rapid, brief fall of energy will excite a nerve at 30° C. and fail to excite one at 4° C.2 Xot only does temperature influence the ability of the nerve to take on the change which is associated with the development of what we call the nerve impulse, but it alters its power of recovery. This appears in experiments in which the ability of the nerve to respond to two rapidly following stimuli is tested by different temperatures. A nerve, like the heart-muscle, shows a " refrac- tory period" for a short interval after excitation, and during this period it is incapable of responding to stimulation. The length of the interval varies with the temperature. If the two stimuli are separated by an interval of 0.001 sec, the second stimulus will be effective at 15° C, but it will fail at 3° C. ; at this temperature, even with an interval of 0.006 sec., the second stimulus will be without effect, as much as 0.01— 0.02 sec. being needed for the recovery of nerve at this low temperature.3 Cold, unless excessive and long continued, though it temporarily suspends, does not destroy the irritability; while heat, if at all great, so alters the chemical constitution of the cell-protoplasm as to destroy its life. The higher the temperature the more rapid the chemical changes of the body and the less its power of resistance ; low temperature, on the other hand, 1 Gotch and Macdonald: Journal <>/ Physiology, 1896, xx. p. 247. 2 Waller: Ibid., 1899, x.xiv. p.L 8 Gotch and Burch: Ibid., 1899, xxiii. |>. 'J-J ; Boycott : Ibid., 1899, xxiv. p. 144. 58 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. .-lows chemical processes and increases the endurance. It is noticeable that aerves and muscles remain irritable much longer than ordinarily in ease the body be cooled before their removal. In tie case of a mammal, the irritability may last from six to eighl hours instead of two and a half, while in the ease of frogs it may be preserved at 0° for ten days, although at summer heat it lasts bnly twenty-four hours. In the case of frogs which have been kept at a low temperature the irritability becomes abnormally high when they are warmed to ordinary room-temperature. Effect of Chemicals and Drugs. — The irritability of nerve and muscle proto- plasm is markedly influenced by even slight changes in its constitution, If a nerve or muscle be allowed to lie in a liquid of a different composition from its own fluid, and especially if such a liquid be injected into its blood-vessels, an interchange of materials takes place which results in an alteration of the i stitution of the tissue and a change in its irritability. Indeed, the only solutions which fail to alter the irritability are those which closely resemble serum and lymph. Fluids having other than the normal percentage of salts have a marked effect, while even the absence of proteids appears to have little influence unless continued for a considerable time. Pure water acts as a poison to protoplasm, soon destroying its life. Through diffusion and osmosis it is imbibed into the cells at the same time that the salts pass out, and the resulting change in the physical and chemical condition of the tissue cause if rapid, first an increase, and in any case later a decrease, and finally a total loss of irritability. Thus water injected into the blood-vessels of muscles first excites contraction and later destroys the irritability, and results in the condition known as water rigor. These effects are prevented by the presence of small amounts of salt. A sodium chloride solution, of a strength of <> parts per 1000 of distilled water, has been called the physiological solution, because it was supposed to have no effect on the irritability of nerves and muscles of cold-blooded animals; even this solution, if long continued, gradually increases and later decreases the irritability. A solution containing 7 parts of sodium chloride per 1000 is more nearly isotonic to the fluids of cells of the frog, and one containing 9 parts per 1000 is approximately in osmotic equilibrium with the fluids of the e ordinary room-temperature. Effect <f irritability caused by weak, medium, and strong battery currents: A and Bindicate the points of application of the electrodes to the nerve, A being the anode, Bthe kathode. The horizontal line represents the nerve at normal irritability; the curved lines illus- trate how the irritability is altered al differenl parts of the nerve with currents of different strengths. curve v1 Bhows the effect ofs weak current, the part below the line indicating decreased, and that above the line increased Irritability, at z* the curve crosses the line, this being the indifferent point at which atelectrotonic effects are compensated for by anelectrotonic ctl'ects: //-gives t lie effect of a stronger fa 8tm stronger current. As the strength of the current is increased the effect becomes r and extend- farther Into the extrapolar regions. In the intrapolar region the indifferent point is to advance f action as a test, have lately discovered that for a brief period after the nerve has been stimulated it is incapable of responding to a second Stimulus. The length of the period of lessened excitability is greatly influ- enced by temperature; at 1 <'.. with maximal stimuli, the "critical period" may lie 0.007-0.008 second ; at higher temperatures it is shorter. (I>) Influences which favor the maintenance of the Normal Physiological Oondiiion <>f Nerve 8. - Cashing: Journal of Physiology, 1897, vol. xxi. p. '_'] 1. 5 Boycott : Ibid., 1899, v<<\. sxiv. p. 144. * Gotch and Burch: Ibid., p. 410. GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. 67 and tissues, aud the circulating blood is a medium of exchange. The blood carries nutritive materials from the digestive organs and oxygen from the lungs to all the tissues of the body, and it transports the waste materials which the cells give off to the excretory organs. In addition to these functions it has the power to neutralize the acids which are produced by the cells during action, and so maintain the alkalinity essential to the life of the cell ; it sup- plies all parts with moisture; by virtue of the salts which it contains, it secures the imbibition relations which are necessary to the preservation of the normal chemical constitution of the cell-protoplasm; it distributes the heat, and so equalizes the temperature of the body; finally, in addition to these and other similar functions, it is itself the seat of important chemical changes, in which the living cells which it contains play an active part. It is not strange that such a fluid should exert a marked influence upon the irritability of the nerves and muscles. Since the metabolism of muscles is best understood, we will first consider the importance of the circulation to the muscle. Muscles, even in the so-called state of rest, are the seat of chemical changes by which energy is liberated, and when they are active these changes may be very extensive. If the cell is to continue its work, it must be at all times in receipt of mate- rials to replenish the continually lessening store of energy-holding compounds; moreover, as the setting free of energy is largely a process of oxidation, a free supply of oxygen is likewise indispensable to action. These oxidation pro- cesses result in the formation of waste products — such as carbon dioxide, water, lactic acid — and these are injurious to the muscle protoplasm, and if allowed to accumulate would finally kill it. Of the services which the blood renders to the muscle there are, therefore, two of paramount importance, viz. the bringing of nutriment and oxygen and the removal of waste matter, and sur- plus energy, as heat. A classical experiment illustrating the effect of depriving tissues of blood is that of Stenson, which consists in the closure of the abdominal aorta of a warm-blooded animal by a ligature, or by compression. In the case of a rabbit, for example, the blood is shut off, not only from the limbs but from the lower part of the spinal cord. The effect is soon manifested in a complete paralysis of the lower extremities, sensation as well as power of voluntary and reflex movements being lost. The paralysis is due, in the first instance, to the loss of function of the nerve-cells iu the cord by which the muscles are nor- mally excited to action. Later, however, the nerves and muscles of the limbs lose their irritability. Of the peripheral mechanisms the motor nerve-ends are found to succumb before the nerves and muscles. This is shown by the fact that although the muscles are still capable of responding to direct irrita- tion, they are not affected by stimuli applied to the nerve, although the nerve at the time, to judge from electrical changes which occur when it is excited, is still irritable. Since the nerve and muscle an1 irritable, the lack of response must be attributed to the nerve-ends. The response to indirect stimulation (i. e. excitation of a muscle by irritating its nerve) is lost in about twenty minutes, while the irritability of the muscle, as tested by direct excitation, is 68 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. not lost for four or five hours. In tin's as in so many instances the loss of irritability of the muscle is due primarily to the disturbance of the respira- tion of the muscle. Of the substances supplied to the muscle by the blood, oxygen is one the want of which is soonest felt. The muscle contains within itself a certain store of oxygen, but one which is by no means equal to the amount of oxidizable substances. Of this oxygen, that which is in the least stable combinations, and which is available for immediate needs, is soon exhausted. A continual supply of oxygen is required even for the chem- ical changes which occur iu the quiet muscle. Of the waste substances which the blood removes from the cell, carbon dioxide is the one which accumu- lates most rapidly and is the first to lessen the irritability. Lactic acid and waste products from the breaking down of nitrogenous materials of the cell are also injurious. The dependence of nerve-fibres upon the blood-supply is by no means so well understood. The nerve-fibre is a branch of a nerve-cell, and it seems as if the nourishment of the fibre was largely dependent upon that of the cell- bodv (see Fatigue of* Nerve, pp. 75 and 95). Nevertheless, the nerve-fibre requires a constant supply of blood -for the maintenance of its irritability. The irritability of the nerve cannot long continue without oxygen, and a nerve which has been removed from the body is found to remain irritable longer in oxygen than in air, and in air than in an atmosphere containing no oxygen. Waste products liberated by active muscles have a deleterious effect on nerves ; whether such substances are produced in the nerves them- selves will be considered later. The efficacy of the blood to preserve the irritability is to be seen in such experiments as those of Ludwig and Schmidt ;* they succeeded in maintaining the artificial circulation of defibrinated, aerated blood through the muscles of a dog, and kept them irritable for many hours after death of the animal. If such an experiment is to be successful, the blood must be maintained at the normal temperature, be plentifully supplied with oxygen, and be kept as free from carbon dioxide as possible. Von Frey2 made an elaborate experiment of this nature. A dog was killed, the body was cut in halves, and the aorta and inferior vena cava were quickly connected with an apparatus for pumping the blood at a regular rate through the hind part of the body. Before the blood entered the arteries it passed through coils in which it was warmed to the nor- mal temperature, and an artificial lung, where it received a supply of oxygen and was relieved of its carbon dioxide. Under these conditions the muscles were kepi alive for more than seven hours, and so far retained their normal condition that throughout this period they were able to respond to stimuli sent to them through their nerves and contract with sufficient vigor to raise a considerable weight. H. X. Martin3 made a similar experiment on the heart 1 SUzungaberichte der math.-phys. Claisse der k. sacks. Gesellschaft der Wissenschaftm, vol. xx., 1868. 2 " Versuche iiber den StoflTweohsel des Muskels," Archiv fur Anatomic und Physiologic, 1885; physiologische AbtheilunK, S. 533. 3 Studies from the Biological Laboratory of Johns Hopkins University, 1882, vol. ii. p. 188. GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. 69 of a dog. The heart and lungs were isolated from the rest of the body, the heart was fed with defibrinated blood from a Mariotte flask, and the Lungs were supplied with air by an artificial respiration apparatus. The heart, which was kept moist and at the normal temperature, continued to beat for four hours and more. Porter1 has succeeded in keeping even small pieces of the ven- tricle of the mammalian heart alive by maintaining a good circulation of well-oxygenated blood through its vessels (see Section on Nutrition of the Heart). Normally the blood-supply to the muscle is varied according to its needs. When the muscle is stimulated to action its blood-vessels are at the same time dilated, so that it receives a free supply of blood.2 Moreover, if mus- cular work is extensive, the heart beats faster and the respiratory movements are quicker, so that a larger amount of oxygen is provided and the carbon dioxide is removed more rapidly. The importance of the blood-supply to a muscle can be best understood if we consider it in relation to the effects of fatiguing work upon the muscles (see p. 74). The relation of special sub- stances in the blood to the needs of the muscle can be best considered together with the chemistry of the muscle (see p. 159). Effect of Separation from the Central Nervous System. — If a motor nerve be cut, or if some part of it be so injured that the fibres lose their power of conduction, the portion of the nerve thus separated from the central nervous system sooner or later completely degenerates (see p. 77). Each of the motor nerve-fibres is a branch of a motor cell in the anterior horns of the spinal cord. These nerve-cells are supposed to govern the nutrition of their processes, though how a microscopic cell can thus influence a nerve-fibre a meter or so long is by no means clear. Soon after the nerve is separated from its cell it exhibits a change of excitability. In general it responds more readily to the kathode than the anode, which would imply an increased irritability; at the part near the cut, however, it responds best to the anode.3 The increase is soon followed by a gradual decrease of irritability. In the ease of mammalian nerves loss of irritability may be complete at the end of three or four days, but the nerves of cold-bl led animal may retain their irri- tability for several weeks. The immediate cause of the loss of irritability is the change in the chemical and physiological structure of the axis-cylinder. The degenerative changes result finally in the complete destruction of the nerve-fibres, and involve the motor end-organs as well, but do not imme- diately invade the muscle, which may be considered a proof that nerve and muscle protoplasm are not continuous. Though no immediate change in the structure of the muscle is observable, the irritability of the muscle soon begins to alter. At the end of a fortnight the irritability of the muscle for all forms of stimuli is lessened. From this time on, the irritability gradually undergoes a remarkable change, the excitability 1 Porter: American Journal of Physiology, 1899, vol. ii. p. 127. 2Sczelkow: Sitzungsbcr. d. k. A had, Wim, 1862, Bel. xlv. Abtli. 1. 3 Blix: Skandinavischcs Archil' fur Physiologic, 1889, Bd. i, S. 184. 70 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. for mechanical irritants and for direct battery currents (see p. 54) beginning to increase, but the power to respond to electric currents of short duration, as induction shocks, continuing to lessen; indeed, the reactions of the muscle appear to take on more of the character of those of smooth muscle- fibres. The condition of increasing irritability to direct battery currents and mechanical irritants reaches its maximum by the end of the seventh week, and from that time on the power to respond to all forms of stimuli lessens, the excitability being wholly lost by the end of the seventh or eighth month. During the stage of increased excitability fibrillary contractions are often observed. As in the case of a nerve, so of the muscle the loss of irritability is due to degenerative changes which gradually lead to the destruction of the muscle protoplasm. The cause of the change in the muscle is still a matter of doubt, some regarding it as due to the absence of some nutritive, trophic influence from the central nervous system, others consider it to be the result of cir- culatory disturbances, consequent upon the lack of a proper regulation of the blood-supply, due to the division of the vaso-motor nerves, and still others attribute it to a lack of exercise, it being no longer stimulated to action. As regards the second view, it may be said that muscles whose vaso-motor nerves are intact, the vessels being innervated through other nerves than those which supply the muscle-tissue proper, as is the case with some of the facial muscles, undergo similar changes in irritability when their motor uerves are cut. As regards the first and last views, it may be said that if the muscles be artificially excited, as by electric stimuli, and thus are exer- cised daily, the coming on of degeneration can be at least greatly delayed. The question as to whether the anabolic processes within the muscle-cell are dependent on the central nervous system, in the sense of their being specific trophic influences sent from the nerve-cells to the muscles, is still under discussion and need not be considered further in this place. Without doubt the reflex tonus impulses which during waking hours are all the time coming to the muscles are productive of katabolic changes and, indirectly at least, favor anabolism. (c) Effect of Influences u-hicJi result from the Functional Activity of Nerves and Muscles. — Fatigue of Muscles. — The condition of muscular fatigue is cha- racterized by lessened irritability, decrease in the rate and vigor with which the muscle contracts and liberates energy, and a still greater decrease in the rate with which it relaxes and recovers its normal form. In a sense, whatever induces such a state can be said to cause fatigue, but it is perhaps best to restrict the term to the form of fatigue which is produced by excessive functional activity. The cause of exhaustion which results from over- work is in part the same as the cause of the loss of irritability and power which follows the cutting off of the blood-supply. The working cell liberates energy at the expense of its store of nutriment and oxygen, and through oxi- dation processes forms waste products which are poisonous to its protoplasm. The fatigue which results from functional activity has, therefore, a twofold GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. 71 cause, the decrease in energy-holding compounds available for work and the accumulation of poisonous waste matters. It is evident that the length of time that the cell can continue to work will depend very much upon the rapidity with which the energy-holding explosive compounds are formed by the cell-protoplasm and the waste products are excreted. If a muscle is made to contract vigorously and continuously, as when a heavy weight is held up, fatigue comes quickly ; on the other hand, a muscle may be contracted a great many times if each contraction is of short duration and considerable intervals of rest intervene between the succeeding contractions. The best illustration of this is the heart, which, though making contractions in the case of man at the rate of seventy or more times a minute, is able to beat without fatigue throughout the life of the individual. Each of the vigorous contractions, or systoles, is followed by an interval of rest, diastole, during which the cells have time to recuperate. The same is true of the skeletal muscles. It was found in an experiment that if a muscle of the hand, the abductor indicis, were contracted at regular intervals, a weight being so arranged that it was lifted by the finger each time the muscle shortened, a light weight could be raised at the rate of once a second for two hours and a half, i. e. more than 9000 times, without any evidence of fatigue. If, however, the weight was increased, which required a greater output of energy, or if the rate of contractions was increased, which shortened the time of repose, the mus- cle fatigued rapidly. In general, the greater the weight which the muscle has to lift, the shorter must be the periods of contraction in proportion to the inter- val of rest if the muscle is to maintain its power to work. Maggiora,1 in his interesting experiments in Mosso's laboratory at Turin, made a very careful study of this subject, and ascertained that for a special group of muscles there is for each individual a definite weight and rate of contraction essential to the accom- plishment of the greatest possible work in a given time. These experiments were made on men, and the height of the succeeding contractions was re- corded by an apparatus devised by Mosso, the ergograph,2 which made it possible to estimate the total amount of work done by the muscles studied. Many forms of apparatus have since been devised to accomplish this. Mosso's ergograph consisted of two parts, an arm rest equipped with suitable clamps for fixing the arm and hand, and a writing mechanism arranged to record the movements of the weight which was raised hv the flexion of the second linger. Either increasing the weight or the rate of contrac- tion hastens the coming on of fatigue and so lessens the power and the total amount of work. In such an exercise as walking the muscles are not continually acting, but intervals of rest alternate with the periods of work, and the time for recuperation is sufficiently long to permit the protoplasm of the muscle-cells t<> prepare the chemical compounds from which the energy is liberated as fast as they are used, and get rid of the 1 Archiv fiir Anatomie uittl Physiologie, 1890; physiologische Abtheilung, S. 191. 2 Mosso: Die Ermiidung, Leipzig, 181*2, S. '.hi ; Lombard: Journal of Physiology, 1892, vol. xiii. Fig. 1, Plate 1. 72 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. waste products of contraction, so that vigorous muscles can be employed many hours before any marked fatigue is experienced. Sooner or later, however, the vigor of the muscle begins to decrease. The reason for this is nut wholly clear. It is noticeable, however, that not only the muscles employed in the work, hut other muscles, such as those of the arms for instance, even when purposely kept quiet, have their irritability reduced. This would suggest that the fatigue which finally asserts itself is due to some general rather than local influence. To understand this we must recall the fact that all parts of the body are in communication by means of the circulatory system. The ever-circulating blood as it is thrown out by the heart is divided into minute streams, which, after passing through the many organs of the body, unite again on their return to the heart. If materials be taken from the blood by one part, they are lost to all the rest, and if materials be added to the blood by any part, they are sooner or later carried to all the rest. During the course of a lung march, the muscles of the leg take up a great deal of nutriment, and give off many waste products, and all the organs suffer in con- sequence. Mosso,1 in his experiments upon soldiers taking long forced marches, found that lack of nutriment is not the only cause of the general fatigue produced by long-continued muscular work. The soldiers, though somewhat refreshed by the taking of food, did not recover completely until after a pro- longed interval of rest. He attributed this to the fatigue-products which he supposed the muscles to have given off, and concluded that they were only gradually eliminated from the blood. To see if there were fatigue-products in the blood of a tired animal capable of lessening the irritability of organs other than those which had been working, he made the following experiment: He drew a certain weight of blood from the veins of a dog, and then put back into the animal an equal amount of blood from another completely rested dog. The dug which was the subject of the experiment appeared to be all right after the operation. On another day he repeated the experiment, but this time the blood which was put back was taken from a dog that was completely tired out by running. The effect of the blood from the fatigued animal was very marked ; the dog receiving it showed all the signs of fatigue, and crept off into a corner to sleep. Mosso concluded from this experiment, that during mus- cular work fatigue-products are generated in the muscles, pass thence into the blood, and are conveyed to other muscles, where they produce the lowered irritability and loss of power characteristic of fatigue. Many years before, Von Ranke extracted from the tired muscles of frogs substances which he considered fatigue materials. Lee2 would draw a sharp distinction between fatigue and exhaustion. lb' considers the former to be a transient change in the capacity for work induced by the presence of waste products, while the latter is a far more serious condition and is due to a lack of nutritive energy-giving substance. He considers that fatigue, by lessening the irri- 1 Arcliir fur Anatomic mul Physiologie, 1890; phvsiolmrische Abtheilung. 2 Proceedings of the American Physiological Society, Dec, 1898, published in American Journal of Physiology, 1899, vol. ii. p. 11. GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. 73 tability, may exert a protective influence and prevent the work from being carried under ordinary conditions to the point of exhaustion. In favor of this view he states that the muscles even of starving animals, although incapable of long-continued work, do not make contractions of the tvpe characteristic of fatigued muscles (see p. 115); on the other hand, muscles which have been subjected to lactic acid, one of the waste products resulting from muscular work, whether it be free or combined, as it probably is in the muscle, with potassium or sodium, do make contractions of the type shown by fatigued muscles. Waller1 has of late laid much stress upon the action of C02 to stimulate protoplasm when present in small amounts and to anaes- thetize it when in larger quantities. C02 is also a waste product of muscle, but it is doubtful whether the paralyzing effect of large amounts can be regarded as a fatigue effect. Maggiora, in his experiments upon the fatigue of special groups of mus- cles, likewise found that the taking of food causes only a partial recovery of the tired muscles, and that an interval of rest is essential to complete recoverv. In these experiments the irritability of the muscles was tested not only by volitional impulses, but by the strength of the electric current required to cause direct excitation. A curve of fatigue of human muscles by voluntary contractions is shown in Fig. 59, and one resulting from electrical excitation of the muscle in Fig. 58. In the case of vigorous men, one and a half hours suffice to restore the muscles of the forearm which have been completelv tired out by raising a heavy weight many times. He also observed that the time required for recovery can be greatly shortened if the circulation of the blood and lymph in the muscles be increased by massage. This suggests that the power of the cell to give off its waste products to the blood is sufficiently rapid to keep pace with the ordinary production, but not with the more rapid formation taking place during fatiguing work. This would seem to be the case in spite of the fact that circulation of the blood and lymph in the mus- cles is increased during action. This increase in the circulation through the acting muscle is brought about in part by the fact that the muscle massages itself by its own contractions. It is a pumping mechanism, which acts at the time when the increased taking of oxygen and nutriment and giving off of waste products make the rapid renewal of the restoring fluids imperative. Every time the muscle contracts the swelling, tense fibres compress the lym- phatics and blood-vessels between and about them, and when it relaxes the valves in the lymph vessels and veins prevent the return of the fluid which has been squeezed out. In addition to this, when muscles are stimulated to action by impulses coming to them from the central nervous system, the mus- cles in the walls of the blood-vessels of the muscle are acted upon by their vaso-dilator nerves, and, relaxing, permit a greater flow of blood through the muscle ; when the muscles cease to be excited the muscles in the vessel walls gradually regain their tone, and the blood-supply to the muscle tissue is correspondingly lessened. This arrangement would seem to suffice for the 1 Lectures on Physiology, first series, on Animal Electricity, London, 1897, p. 47. 74 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OE PHYSIOLOGY. bringing of nutriment and oxygen and the removal of waste matters under ordinary conditions. Considerable difference of opinion exists as to which of three classes of food-stuffs — proteids, carbohydrates, and fats — supply the energy used by the muscle in ordinary and excessive work, and how these are employed by the muscle. The question has been studied by examining the character and quantity of waste products liberated from the body during and after excessive mus- cular work, as compared with those given off" when the subject is at rest. Another method has been to test the strength of the muscle in ergographic experiments, and to find the effect of different kinds of food upon the time required for its recovery. Experiments of Kick and Wislicenus,1 Voit and Pettenkofer,2 Voit,3 and others caused the view to become generally accepted that the energy of the muscle by violent muscular work comes largely from the non-proteid substances in the muscles. Later Pfluger and his pupils have gone to the other extreme and conclude that proteid is the chief source of energy.1 Very many others have written on both sides of the subject and still a final conclusion has not been reached.5 Probably the sugars, and possibly after these the fats are employed by the muscle as the most available form of energy, while the proteid forms a more permanent part of the muscular machine, and is only made use of when the work is exhaustive (see page 166). The taking of any one of these classes of food hastens the recovery from fatigue, and the sooner the more readily it is digested and assimilated (see Metabolism — effect of muscular work). Normally the muscles are never completely fatigued. It would seem that as the muscles tire and their irritability is lessened, the central nerve- cells which send the stimulating impulses to them have to work harder, and that the nerve-cells give out sooner than the muscles. On the other hand, certain experiments seem to show that the nerve-cells recover from fatigue more rapidly than the muscles do, so that it is an advantage to the organism that they should cease to excite the muscles before muscular fatigue is complete. With the decreasing irritability of the muscle, a feeling of discomfort in the muscle and an increasing sense of effort are experienced by the individual, both of which tend to cause a cessation of contraction, and prevent a harmful amount of work. That such an arrangement would be of service was apparent in the experiments of Maggiora, in which he found that if muscles are forced to work after fatigue has developed, the time of recovery is prolonged out of all proportion to the extra work accomplished. At the close of even exhaustive muscular work there is always a large amount of energy-holding materials in the blood and tissues, and the rapid, 1 Vierteljahrexschrift der naturforsche Gesclkchafl in Zurich, 1865, Bd. X. S. 317. 2 Zeilschnfl fur Biologic, 1866, Bd. ii. 3 Ibid., 1876, Bd. vi. S. 305. 1 Pfluger' s Arclav, 1899, Bd. 77, S. 425. s Schafer's Text-book of Physiology, 1898, vol. i. p. 912. GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. 75 though partial, improvement in the condition on the taking of food is per- haps best explained as the result of a stimulating effect on the central nerv- ous system. This might be due to the change in the circulation which follows the taking of food, as well as the fact that a fresh supply of uncombined and hence available energy-holding substances is being received. The effect of the so-called stimulants, alcohol, tea, coffee, etc., to temporarily increase the ability to do work, is probably chiefly through their action on the central nervous system. Their influence is a temporary one, and only markedly increases the amount of work when the body has a plentiful supply of nutri- ment.1 Fatigue of Nerves. — Muscle-, gland- and nerve-cells — in fact, almost every form of protoplasm — if excited to vigorous long-continued action, deteri- orate and exhibit a decline of functional activity. As we have seen, in the case of muscle there are a using up of available energy-holding compounds and a production of poisonous waste matters, and these two effects induce the condition known as fatigue. A priori, we should expect similar changes to occur in the active nerve-fibre ; almost all the experimental evidence is, how- ever, opposed to this view. The form of activity which is most character- istic of muscle is contraction ; that which is most characteristic of nerve is conduction. In the case of the muscle it is exceedingly difficult to distin- guish between the effects produced by the processes associated with the change of form and those which result from the transmission of the excitatory change. There is little doubt that fatigue is associated with the former ; whether it is associated with the latter is not known. In the case of the nerve, where the transmission process may be studied by itself, conduction does not seem to fatigue (see p. 95). Apparently the same may be said of the processes which result in the development of what we call the nerve-impulse. We have already seen that the nerve may undergo an alteration of irritability if subjected to artificial irritants. Such a change at the point of application of the irritant is hardly to be regarded as a fatigue effect, however, for in many cases, at least, it is due to the direct effect of the irritant on the physical or chemical structure of the nerve-protoplasm rather than to molecular changes which are peculiar to the development of the nerve-impulse. Thus the change of irritability which results from a series of light blows, such as may be given to a nerve by Tigerstedt's tetanomotor, cannot properly be said to be the result of fatigue. It has been found that a medullary nerve may be excited many times a second for hours, by an induced current, and still be capable of developing at the stimulated point what we call the nerve-impulse. The change which is de- veloped at the point of excitation and which passes thence the length of the nerve, would seem to be the expression of a form of energy liberated within the nerve, and since the liberation of energy implies the breaking down of chemical combinations, the apparent lack of fatigue of the nerve is incompre- hensible. It is the more remarkable since the nerve-fibre is to be considered a 1 Schumburg: Archil) JUr Anatomic und Physiologie, 1899, supplement, S. '289. 76 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. branch of a nerve-cell, and nerve-cells appear to fatigue if frequently excited to vigorous action. Inasmuch as we have as yet no definite knowledge of the nature of what we call the nerve-impulse, or of the character of the processes by which it is transmitted along the nerve, we can afford to leave this question open, and simply state that the evidence thus far obtained is opposed to the view that nerve-fibres fatigue. Effect of Use and Disuse. — Different kinds of muscle-tissues possess very different degrees of endurance. By endurance we mean the capacity to liber- ate energy during long periods of time. This capacity is intimately associated with irritability, for one of the first marks of failure of power is a decline of irritability. In general, the more irritable a muscle the less its endurance, because with an increase of irritability there is associated a more rapid and extensive liberation of energy in response to irritants. For example, the rap- idly responding and acting pale striated muscles of the rabbit have less resist- ing power than the red striated muscles, while the sluggish unstriated muscle- fibres can contract a long time without suffering from fatigue. The endurance of muscles of even the same kind may differ very considera- bly in the same individual, but the differences are more striking in the case of different individuals. One of the causes of this is the extent to which the muscles are employed. Use, exercise, is the most effective method of increasing not only the strength, but the endurance of the muscle. Though this fact is so well known as to scarcely need repeating, the explanation of it is by no means so clear. Undoubtedly one of the causes is a more perfect circulation in a muscle which is often used, but this is not all. It would seem as if the protoplasm of the muscle-cell was educated, so to speak, to be more expert in assimilating materials containing energy, in building up the explosive compounds emploved in its work, and in excreting deleterious waste matters. The effect of exercise upon irritability has not been thoroughly worked out. It would seem as if there were a normal degree of irritability for each special form of muscle-tissne, and as if either an increase or decrease of the irritability above or below this level was a sign of deterioration. Exercise, if not excess- ive, is favorable to the maintenance of this normal physiological condition. Without doubt many of the differences which we attribute to the muscles of different men are really due to differences in the central nerve-cells, the action of muscles, rightly interpreted, being rather an expression of central nervous activity than the result of peculiarities of the muscles themselves. To vol- untarily exercise the muscles is to exercise the nerve-cells, and the effects of exercise upon these nervous mechanisms is of as much importance as the effect upon the muscles. In admiring visible proportions we must always bear in mind that though "beef" is of use to the athlete, the muscles are merely the servants, and can accomplish nothing if the master is sick. The nerve-cells always give out before the muscles, and the man preparing for a contest should watch his nervous system more than his muscles. He who forgets this can easily over-train, and do himself a permanent injury, besides failing in the race. GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. 77 Effect of Enforced Rest. — Not only is the strength of the muscles greatly increased by exercise, but a lack of exercise soon results in a loss of strength. This is seen when an individual is confined to his bed for even a comparatively short time, or when a limb is subjected to enforced rest by being placed in a splint. The cause is to be sought in changes peculiar to the muscle proto- plasm itself, although reduced circulation may also play a part. The effect of prolonged rest on the irritability of muscles, is seen most markedly when they are separated from the central nervous system by injuries of their nerves (see p. 70). The lowered irritability which results from prolonged rest is not peculiar to muscles, but is shared by all forms of protoplasm. C. Conductivity. Conductivity is that property of protoplasm by virtue of which a condition of activity aroused in one portion of the substance, by the action of a stimulus of any kind, may be transmitted to any other portion. For example, if the edge of the bell of a vorticella (see Fig. 2, p. 19) be irritated by a hair, not only do the movements of the cilia cease, but the contractile substance of the bell draws it into a more compact shape, and the fibrilhe of the stalk shorten and pull the bell away from the offending irritant. In such a case an exciting process must have been transmitted throughout the cell, and through several more or less differentiated forms of protoplasm. This property of conductivity is not known to be limited to any one peculiar structural arrangement of protoplasm distinguishable with the microscope, but is exhibited by a vast variety of forms of cell-protoplasm, and by plants as well as animals. The cytoplasm of cells, the part of the protoplasm surrounding the nucleus, appears to be composed of a semifluid granular material, traversed in all directions by finest fibrillar which in some cases appear to form an irregular meshwork, the reticulum, and in others to be arranged side by side as more or less complete fibrils. It is not known whether the power of conduction is possessed by the whole of the pro- toplasmic substance or is confined to the reticular substance, but there are cer- tain reasons why the former view may be considered the more probable. The rate and the strength of the conduction process varies greatly in different forms of protoplasm, and there appear to be differences in the facility with which the exciting process spreads through different parts of even the same cell.1 Not only are such differences to be noticed in many of the ciliated infusoria, but even the substance of striated muscles seems to conduct in two different ways, the sarcoplasm appearing to conduct slowly, and the more highly differentiated fibrillary portion of the fibre rapidly. In general the process appears to be more rapid and vigorous where a fibrillated structure is observable. Smooth muscle-tissue, which has a somewhat simple structure, conducts comparatively slowly; striated muscle, which is more highly differentiated, more rapidly, and the fibrillated axis-cylinder of the ncrvc-libre, most rapidly of all. Protoplasmic Continuity is Essential to Conduction. — Effect of a Break in Protoplasmic ( 'ontinmby.- — A break of protoplasmic continuity in any 1 Biedermann : Elektrophysiologic, 1895, 8. 137. 78 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. part of a nerve- or muscle-fibre acts as a barrier to conduction. If a nerve be cut through, the irritability and conductivity remain for a considerable time in the severed extremities, but communication between them is lost, and remains absent however well the cut extremities may be adjusted. The nerve-impulse is not transmitted through the nerve-substance as electricity is transmitted along a wire: join the cut ends of a wire, and the contact suffices for the passage of the current ; join the cut ends of a nerve, and the nerve-impulse cannot pass. Any severe injury to a nerve alters the protoplasmic structure and prevents the chemical and physical processes through which conductivity is made possible. It is probable that the same may be said of all forms of liv- ing cells, and the absence of protoplasmic continuity would seem to be an explanation of the fact that nerve- and muscle-fibres which lie close together may physiologically act as separate mechanisms. Even in the case of apparently homogeneous protoplasm there is probably a definite structural relation of the finest particles, and upon this the physi- ological properties of the substance depends. Slight physical and chemical alterations suffice to change the rate and strength of the conduction process, and the power to conduct is altogether lost if the protoplasm is so altered that it dies. The relation of conductivity to structure of cell-protoplasm is illustrated in the effects of degeneration and regeneration upon the physiological properties of the nerve-fibre (see p. 69). The life of the nerve-fibre is dependent on influ- ences exerted upon it by the body of the cell of which it is a branch. When any part of the fibre is injured it loses its power to conduct, and the portion of the fibre separated by this block from the body of the cell soon dies. The irritability and conductivity are wholly lost at the end of a period varying from four davs to several weeks, the time differing in different kinds of nerves, and the fibre begins to undergo degeneration. The axis-cylinder and the myelin are seen to break up and are then absorbed, apparently with the assistance of the nuclei which normally lie just inside the neurilemma, and which at this time proliferate greatly and come to occupy most of the lumen of the tube. The process of absorption is nearly complete at the end of a fortnight after the injuy. Under suitable conditions, however, regeneration may occur, and as this takes place there is a recovery of physi- ological activities. The order in which conductivity and irritability return is instructive. Howell and Huber1 made a careful study of this subject. They found that the many nuclei which develop during degeneration are apparently the source of new protoplasm, which is seen to accumulate in the old sheath until a continuous band of protoplasm is formed. About this thread of protoplasm a new membranous sheath develops, and thus is built up what closely resembles an embryonic nerve-fibre. The embryonic fibre formed in the peripheral end of the regenerating nerve joins that of the central end in the cicatricial tissue which has been deposited at the point of injury. Thus a temporary nerve- fibre is formed and united to the undegenerated part of the old fibre, and this 1 Journal of Physiology, 1892, vol. xiii. p. 381. GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. 79 new structure, though possessing neither myelin nor axis-cylinder, i> found to be capable of conduction and to have a low form of irritability, being ex- citable to violent mechanical stimuli but not to induction currents. The power of conduction appears to return before irritability, and may be observed first at the end of the third week. Apparently sensation is recovered before the power of making voluntary movements; this difference may well be due, not to any essential difference between sensory and motor fibres, but to the tact that extra time is required for the motor fibres to make connection with the muscle. The embryonic fibre gradually gives place to the adult fibre, new myelin being formed all along the fibre, and a new axis-cylinder growing down from the old axis-cylinder. As the axis-cylinder grows down, the irritability for induction shocks is recovered. Many months may be necessary for the complete recovery of function. Langley ' reports that medullated fibres of the sympathetic, if cut, regenerate and recover the power to function before they regain a medullary sheath. Such experiments show the axis-cylinder to be the true conducting medium, and that the medullary sheath has a sub- ordinate function. The same is true of muscle as of nerve protoplasm, — the power of con- duction ceases with the life of the cell-substance; thus, if the middle part of a muscle-fibre be killed, by pressure, heat, or some chemical, the dead proto- plasm acts as a block to prevent the state of activity which may be excited at one end from being transmitted to the other, and the conduction power is only recovered on the regeneration of the injured tissue. Isolated Conduction is the Rule. — (a) Conduction in Nerve-tmt/nks. — The axis-cylinders of the many fibres which run side by side in a nerve-trunk are separated from each other by the neurilemma, and in the case of the medullary nerves by the myelin substance as well, so that there is not even contiguity, much less continuity of nerve-substance. Thus the many fibres of a nerve- trunk, some afferent and others efferent, though running side by side, conduct independently of one another. For example, if the skin of the foot be pricked, the excitation of its sense-organs is communicated to sensory nerve-fibres, and is transmitted along them to the spinal cord, where the stimulus awakens cer- tain groups of cells to activity; these cells in turn, by means of their branches, the motor nerve-fibres, transmit the condition of excitation down to the mus- cle-fibres of the legs, which, when stimulated, contract and withdraw the foot from the offending irritant. The sensory and motor nerves concerned in this reflex act run for a considerable part of their course in the same nerve-trunk, but the sensory impulses have no direct effect on the motor nerve-fibres, and the roundabout course which has been described is the only way by which they can influence them. Isolated conduction by separate fibres and their branches holds good within the central nervous system, as elsewhere, otherwise we could scarcely explain the localization of sensations, or co-ordinated movements. The presence of a medullary sheath is not essential to isolated conduction, 1 Journal of Physiology, IS'.'T, vol. xxii. p. '2-A. 80 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OE PHYSIOLOGY. for it occurs in the absence of this sheath, both in the peripheral nerves and in the central nervous system. The large class of non-medullated nerves have the power of isolated conduction, and Donaldson reports that new-born rats can make co-ordinated movements, although the nerves of both the peripheral and central nervous systems do not acquire a medullary sheath until several days after birth. It is not likely that the neuroglia cells are essential to isolated conduction within the central nervous system, for this occurs in its absence in the peripheral nerves. Although the neurilemma, by separating the axis-cylinders of adjacent fibres, may make the insulation more complete, it is probably not the real cause of isolated conduction. A break of the protoplasmic continuity of the nerve protoplasm stops conduction, and conduction fails wherever protoplasmic continuity is lacking. An apparent contradiction to the rule that absolute continuity of nervous matter is essential to conduction by nerves, is to be found in the phenomenon known as " Hering's Paradoxical Contraction." This will be explained later (see p. 157, d). (b) Distribution of Excitation by Branches of Nerves. — Nerve-fibres some- times branch in their passage along the peripheral nerves, but most of the branches which are seen to be given off from the nerve-trunks are composed of bundles of nerve-fibres which have merely separated off from the rest. After the nerves have entered a peripheral organ, or the central nervous system, the axis-cylinders may give off branches. Thus in muscles, and toastill greater degree in the electric organs of certain fish, the nerve-fibre and its axis-cylinder may divide again and again, or after entering the spinal cord the fibre may be seen to give off a great many lateral branches — collaterals, as they are called. It is not known whether in such cases the fibrillse of the axis-cylinder give off branches, or whether they simply separate, a part of them entering the branch while the rest of them continue on in the main fibre. Though the exciting process does not pass from fibre to fibre, it probably involves in a greater or less degree all the elements of the same fibre, and passes into all its branches. It is evident that where it is necessary for the irritation to be localized, branching could not occur; but where a more general distribution is permissible, especially where several parts of an organ ought to act at the same instant, conduction through a single fibre which branches freely near its termination would be useful. (c) Conduction in Muscles. — Each fibre of the muscles which move the bones — the skeletal muscles, as they are sometimes called — is physiologically independent of the rest. The sarcolemma prevents not only continuity, but contiguity of the muscle-substance of the separate fibres, and there is no cross conduction from fibre to fibre. That this is so is proved by an experiment, such as was described on page 15, in which unipolar excitation of the part of the fibres of a curarized sartorius muscle results in a contraction strictly con- fined to the fibres which are subjected to the irritating current. Each of the separate muscle-fibres is supplied by at least one nerve-fibre or a branch of a fibre, and, under normal conditions, only acts when stimulated by GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. 81 the nerve. In the case of plant-cells, and of certain forms of mus- cle-cells, about which there is a more or less definite wall or sheath, there are little bridges of protoplasm binding the cells together. For example, Engelmann describes the muscle of the intestine-- of the fly as composed of striated cells, sheathed by sarcolemma, excepl where bound together by little branches of sarcoplasma, which may ad as conducting wires between the cells. There are certain cells, however, which have been supposed to be exceptions to the rule that protoplasmic continuity is essential to conduction. The stri- ated muscle-fibres of the heart are quite different from those of ordinary skeletal muscles, physiologically as well as anatomically. They are stumpy, quadrangular cells, which arc not known to have a sarcolemma, and which are united not only by their broad ends, but by lateral branches. Engelmann and lately Porter1 and others have concluded that conduction take- place in the heart from cell to cell, without the intervention of nerves, and may occur in all directions. This question is considered at length in the section on the conduction of excitation in the heart. The cells of the contractile substance of some of the medusae (as Aurelia), have been supposed to communicate by contiguity rather than by continuity. The same has been thought to be the case with many forms of unstriated muscle-tissue;2 moreover, there are groups of ciliated cells, the members of which act in unison although they have not been found to be connected either directly or by nerves. These cells have apparently no membranous covering, and though living as independent units, are so related that a condition of activity excited in one seems to be transmitted to the resl by means of contact, or through the mediation of cement-sul stance. From what has been said it will be seen that protoplasmic continuity ensures free communication between different cells; that protoplasmic con- tiguity, either directly or through the mediation of the cement-substance, may possibly permit of conduction ; but that normally the intervention of a dif- ferent tissue, even as deli, ate as the sarcolemma, suffices to cause complete isolation of the cell from its neighbors. Under normal conditions there may be a spread of excitation from muscle-fibre to muscle-fibre, even in the skel- etal muscles. Kuhne's experiment with the sartorius muscle of the frog, described on page 45, gives a g 1 proof that the activity of a striated muscle-fibre is not normally transmitted to its neighbors ; nevertheless, Kiihne3 has found that if the extremities of two sartorius muscles be pressed firmly together by a suitable clamp, care being taken that the pressure shall not be enough to destroy the physiological activity of the protoplasm, excitation of one muscle may cause contraction of the other. A satisfactory explanation is lacking. Biedermann ' reported that when a frog's muscle was partly dried a -light 1 Porter: Journal of Experimental Medicine, 1897, ii. p. 891 ; American Jon,,,,,! of Physi- ology, 1899, ii. i». 127. ' Engelmann : Pfttger'i Arch,,; is:i. Bd iv. ' * Kiihne: Untersnchin>,en am der phy iohgUchen hutttvte in Heidelberg, 1880, Bd. 3, S. 1. * Biedermann: Berichte ;• ]\'i,„a- Abulrmic, 1888, Bd. 97, Al.tli. 3, B. 1 !•">. Vol. II -6 82 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. mechanical excitation of one part of it might lead to a contraction of the whole. Similarly, a partly dried-up frog may be seen, if mechanically excited, to make movements simulating life. The cause of these movements, also, is not understood. Drying of the muscle in its early stages greatly increases its irritability because of the concentration of the salts, but that does not account for the loss of insulation. Transmission of Excitation by Means of End-organs. — In spite of the rapid advances which have hem made in the histology and physiology of the nervous system during the past few years, we are still in doubt as to the exact way that the axone, the exciting branch of the neurone, stimulates the cell to which it is distributed. In many cases, at least, the axone terminates in an end-organ which is physiologically dif- j ferent from the rest of the cell, and this end-organ is the exciting agent. The relation of the protoplasm of the end- organ to the protoplasm of the cell which it stimulates, whether one of continuity or contiguity, is not certain, but most histological and physiological observa- Fig. 31. Nerve-termination in voluntary tions are distinctly in favor of the latter muscle of the rabbit, stained in methylen-blue vitam), fixed, sectioned, and counter- View, stained in alum carmin. A, surface view; B, The physiology of the end-organs of longitudinal section through nerve tormina- .. .. ' tion and muscle-fibre; C, cross-section; S, motor axones distributed to striated sarcolemma; n. I., neurilemma. (From Text- muscles is best known book oj Histology, Bohm and Davidoff, revised by '■ < Huber W. B. Saunders, Philadel- Fig. 31 sllOWS a surface View and pina, 1900). a longitU(ljnal an(] cross-section of the end-organ of an axone supplying a voluntary muscle of a rabbit. The axis- cylinder loses its medullary sheath shortly before reaching the fibre, and the neurilemma becomes continuous with the sarcolemma, so that the axis-cylin- der on penetrating the sarcolemma comes into direct contact with the sarco- plasma of the muscle. The sarcoplasroa is heaped together at this place, making a little mound, and the axis-cylinder, after dividing into a number of fine terminal twigs, vmh in the midst of this mass of sarcoplasma. Evi- dently the nerve and muscle protoplasm come into very close relation. On the other hand, nerve and muscle protoplasm retain each its peculiar reaction to staining-fluids, and as far as these chemical reactions can show each main- tain- it- peculiar chemical and histological structure. Moreover, the results of physiological experimentation have shown that, although no definite histo- logical boundary has been found between the axone and its terminal organ, the exciting organ must be considered to be a specially differentiated struct- ure, differing widely from the rest of the neurone. The motor end-organ uses up more time in the excitation of the muscle than would be required for transmission of the excitation through a like amount of nerve- or muscle-substance. It is found by experiment that a GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. 83 muscle does not contract so quickly if it be excited through its nerve as when stimulated directly. Part of the lost time is spent in transmission of the excitation through the nerve ; but after allowance has been made for this loss there is a balance to be accounted for, and this is credited to the motor end- organ. The time used by the motor end-plate is found to be 0.0032 second.1 Motor end-organs are, as we have seen, poisoned by curara (see p. 26) and a number of other drugs which have little influence on the rest of the axone or on the muscle. If a muscle is continuously excited for a considerable time by irritants applied to its nerve, it will at last cease to contract. Direct excitation shows that, though weakened, it is still capable of contraction, and we know that the nerve-fibre does not fatigue. The cessation of contraction is due to fatigue of the motor ends. The motor end-organ is found to lose its vitality quicker than the muscle or the nerve-fibre, if it be deprived of its normal blood-supply. If a motor nerve be cut, the part of the axone separated from the body of the nerve-cell and the terminal organ degenerates, but the degeneration proc- ess stops at the muscle. These facts show that the motor end-organ differs physiologically in many respects from the rest of the axone and from the muscle. Moreover, they favor the idea that excitation is not conducted directly from nerve to muscle protoplasm. That this is the case is also made probable by the fact that though a condition of excitation is transmitted in both directions through nerve and muscle protoplasm as long as there is continuity, a condition of excitation in muscle substance does not appear to be transmitted to the motor nerve. Ap- parently the protoplasm of the end-organ and the muscle are in contact, but are not physiologically continuous, and excitation of muscle protoplasm by the end-organ occurs through some special process. Various views have been advanced with reference to the probable nature of such a process, but as no one of them has received general acceptance they need not be dwelt upon here. One point more, of interest in this connection, is the fact thai it is the sarco- plasma rather than the fibrillary elements of the muscle that conies in contact with the nerve end-organ, which would seem to show that this substance is capable of being excited and conducting the excitation. If this be true of muscle substance, it is likely that the semi-fluid part of the protoplasm of the nerve, as well as perhaps the fibrillary part, may have the power of conduction. As a result of a series of* remarkable histological investigations on the anatomy of the nervous system, the view has come to be generally accepted, that the afferent nerve-fibres entering the spinal cord do not communicate directly with the nerve-cells, but terminate in brush-like endings in close contact with some part of the cells which they excite. A similar arrange- ment has been found wherever nerve-cells are excited to action by nerve- fibres. As in the case of the motor end-organ, it has remained a matter of doubt whether the brush like ends of the axones should be considered to be 1 Bernstein: Arehiv fur Anatomic una Physiologie, 1882, S. 329. 84 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. in contact with the bodies and dendrites of the cells to be excited, and whether this relation would be sufficiently close for a transmission of excitation, or whether they should be considered as specially differentiated exciting mechan- isms, which do not simply transmit the condition of excitation by a proc- ess of conduction, but which develop a special form of physiological stimulus, and through this excite the second neurone to activity. Of late, certain histologists claim to have traced the fibrillae of the axone of one neurone into the cell-body of another neurone, and have even suggested that the nerve impulse from the first might be transmitted through the cell- body of the second and into its branches without the intervention of the pro- toplasm of the body of the cell. It is possible that in some eases the axone of the exciting neurone may, instead of ending close to the neurone to be excited, penetrate it and end in its substance, just as the motor end-organ penetrates into the sarcoplasma of the muscle-fibre. This could happen, and yet the protoplasms of the two cells might preserve" their individuality. There are many facts which show that, physiologically at least, the two neurones act as wholly independent mechanisms. These will be dealt with more at length in the section devoted to the physiology of the central nervous system. Suffice it to say, the end-brush at the extremity of the axone can excite the cell body of another neurone, but cannot be excited by it. A reflex act involving only two neurones requires more time than could be used in simple conduction through the two cells. The character of the impulse sent out of the spinal cord by the efferent cell may be very different from that passing in along the afferent cell — e. g., the efferent impulse may be stronger than the afferent ; the strength of efferent discharge may vary greatly within short intervals of time even when the strength of the afferent impulses remains the same ; Aveak afferent impulses may. by summation, lead to a strong efferent discharge, and continuous afferent stimulation may awaken rhythmic efferent discharges. In short, phvsiological facts are all opposed to the idea that there is con- tinuity of protoplasm of different nerve-cells, and in favor of the view that the end-brush, like the motor end-plate, acts as a specialized exciting mechanism. Conduction in Both Directions. — (a) In Muscle. — Wherever proto- plasmic continuity exists, conductivity would seem to be possible; moreover, the active change excited by an irritant would seem to be able to pass in all directions, though whether with the same facility is not known. Where the spread of the excitatory process is accompanied by a change in form, as is the case in many of the lower organisms and in muscle-tissue, it is not difficult to trace the process. The rate at which the excitation spreads through the irrita- ble substance is very rapid, and special arrangements have to be employed to follow it, but the change is not so swift that its course cannot be accurately determined. It has been found that if a muscle-fibre be stimulated, as nor- mally, by a nerve-fibre, the active condition produced at the point of stimula- tion spreads along the muscle-fibre in both directions to its extremities ; if the GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. 85 fibre be artificially irritated at either end, the exciting change runs the length of the fibre, regardless of the direction, and stimulates every pari of it to con- traction. (6) In Nerves. — In the cases of nerves where excitation is accompanied by no visible manifestation of activity, a definite answer to the question i> not so readily obtained. As long as a nerve is within the normal body, the activity of the nerve-fibre can only be estimated from the response of the cell which the nerve-fibre excites, and there is such an organ onlyal one extremity of the fibre. Paul Bert made a well-known experiment, in which he tried t<> reverse a sensorv nerve in the living animal. He succeeded in bringing about union of the end of the tail of a rat with the tissues of the back, and found, when the union was complete, alter the tail was cut off at its base, it was still capa- ble of giving sensations of pain. The experiment tailed to throw light on the problem, however, for we now know that the peripheral part of the cut nerve dies, and the conduction power manifested in this case was dependent on new axis-cylinders which had grown down from the central nerve-stump (see p. 79). Efforts have been made to elucidate the problem by attempting to unite the central part of a cut sensory nerve with the peripheral part of a divided motor nerve, and observing, after the healing was complete, whether excita- tion of the sensorv nerve caused movements in the pari supplied by tie' motor nerve. Most of these experiments have given doubtful results, but lately Budgett and Green ' have succeeded where others have failed, ami have made cut sensory fibres grow down the degenerated trunk of a motor nerve, and connect with muscle-fibres, so that the muscle contracted when the peripheral end of the sensory fibres was stimulated. The impulse wenl up the old sensorv fibres, and then down the newly developed fibres in the old motor trunk. Their method was to cut the left pneumogastric nerve between the ganglion and the cranium, and to suture its peripheral cut end to the peripheral cut end of the hypoglossal. All the fibres of the hypoglossal and the efferent fibres of the pneumogastric must have degenerated, because these fibres were separated from the bodies of the cells of' which they were branches. The sensory fibres of the pneumogastric, on the other hand, be- cause still in connection with the nerve-cells of the ganglion, continued to live, and tin; part connected with the peripheral -tump of the cut hypoglossal grew down this nerve and came into relation with the muscles of the tongue. Two or three months after the operation the left pneumogastric was divided just above the thorax, and the combined vago-hypoglossal nerve, together with tin; tongue, was excised. When the peripheral end of the pneumogastric was excited the muscles of the tongue were seen t<> contract. Mechanical as well as electrical stimuli were effective, and there would seem to be no escape from the conclusion thai tin- sensory fibres of the pneumogastric had con- ducted the impulse ecntripctally as far as the ganglion, and t hen centrifugally down to the muscle of the tongue. 1 Budgett and (ircen : American Journal of Physiology, 1S99, iii. p. 1 15. 86 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. There is, however, an entirely different method of experimentation which seems to prove that nerve-, like muscle-protoplasm, can conduct in both direc- tion-. This method is based on the fact that though nerve-fibres rarely branch in the peripheral nerve-trunks on their way to an organ, they may divide very freely alter reaching it. Such branchings of fibres occur in muscle, and Kuehne1 found that if one of these branches was stimulated, the irritation passed up the branch to the nerve-fibre and then down the other branches to the muscle. For example, he split the end of the sartorius muscle of a frog by a longitudinal cut, and then found on exciting one of the slips that the other contracted (see Fig. 32). Since cross conduction between striated muscle-fibres does not occur, no other explanation presents itself. Perhaps a still more striking example is to be found in an experiment of Babuchin 2 on the nerve of the electric organ of an electric fish, the Malopterurus. The organ, consisting of many thousand plates, is supplied by a single enormous nerve- fibre which after entering the organ divides very freely so Fig. 82.— Kuehne's ° f . . , . preparation of sarto- as to supply every plate. In this case mechanical stimu- fho* ,1"uble lation of the central end of one of the cut branches of the conduction in nerve. nerve, sufficed to cause an electric discharge of the whole organ. The irritation must have passed backward along the irritated branch until the main trunk was reached and then in the usual direction down the other branches to the electric plates. Still another method is that which was employed by Du Bois-Reymond,3 on the fibres of the spinal nerve-roots. When a nerve is excited to action it undergoes a change in electrical condition, and this change progresses along the fibre at the same rate and in same direction as the nerve-impulse. This electrical change, though entirely different from the nerve-impulse itself, can be taken as an indication of the direction of movement of the process of conduction. Du Bois-Reymond found that if he stimulated the afferent fibres of the posterior spinal nerve-roots of the sciatic nerve of the frog, a "nega- tive variation current," as the current resulting from the change in the elec- trical condition of the nerve is called, passed down the nerve in a direction opposite to that which the normal impulse takes. Further, it was found that if the sciatic oerve was excited, a negative variation current could be detected in the anterior a- well as the posterior roots. Normally the irritation only passes up the posterior roots and down the anterior, for normally the sensory fibres of the posterior roots are excited only at the peripheral end and the motor fibres of the anterior roots only at the central end. The experiment showed both sensory and motor fibres to be capable of conducting in both directions. Normally a nerve is stimulated only at one end, and therefore conducts in only one direction. 1 Archir fur Anatomie und Physiologie, 1859, S. 595. 2 IbuL, is? 7, S 262. 3 Tkierische Electncitdt, 1849, Bd. ii. S. 587. GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. 87 Rate of Conduction. — The activity of the conduction process varies greatly in differenl tissues. The nerves of* warm-blooded animals conducl more rapidly than those of cold ; in a given animal the nerve-fibres conduct more rapidly than muscle-fibres; striated muscle conducts more rapidly than smooth muscle; and even within a single cell different portions may transmit the ex- citing process at different rates ; thus the myoid substance of the contractile fibres of one of the rhizopods conducts more rapidly than the less highly differen- tiated protoplasm of the cell. In general, it may be said that, "the power to conduct increases with increase of mobility and sensitiveness to external irri- tants, a fact which reveals itself in the protozoa, by a comparison of the slowly moving rhizopods with the lively flagellata and ciliata."1 A study of different classes of muscle-tissue supports this view. (a) Rate of Conduction in Muscles. — The conduction process is invisible, hence we estimate its strength and rate by its effects. It is most readily fol- lowed in such mechanisms as muscle, where the conducting medium itself undergoes a change of form as the exciting influence passes along it. Rate of Transmission of ]\'ar<- of Contraction. — If a muscle be excited to action by an irritant applied to one end, it does not contract at once as a whole, but the change of form stalls at the point which is irritated and spreads thence the length of the fibres. At the same time that the muscle shortens it thickens, and under certain conditions the swelling of the muscle can be seen to travel from the end which is excited to the further extremity. In the case of normal, active, striated muscle, the rate at which the change of form spreads over the muscle is far too rapid to be followed by the eye, and hence the muscle appears to act as a whole. By suitable recording mechanisms, evidence can be obtained of the rate at which the exciting influence and contraction pro- cess pass along the fibre. Thus two levers can be so placed as to rest on the two extremities of a muscle, at the same time that the free ends of the levers touch a revolving cylinder, the surface of which is covered with paper black- ened with lampblack. The writing-point of one lever must be directly under the point of the other. If, when the cylinder is revolving, one end of the mus- cle be stimulated, the record will show that the lever resting on that part is the first to move, making it evident that that part of the mus- cle begins to thicken first, and that the contraction docs not begin al the further extremity of the mus- cle until somewhal later. The re- cord given in Figure 33 was ob- tained in a similar experiment, but one in which the contraction of the muscle was registered by the pince myographique and recording tambour of Many (see Fig. 34) 1 Biedermann: Eleklrophysiologie, 1895, Bd. i. S. 124. Fig. 3:i. — Rate of conduction of the contraction pro- cess along ■■' muscle, as Bhown by the difference In the tlmi of thickening of the t\\<> extremities. The tuning* f«.rk waves record ,\„ second (after Marey), 88 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. Bernstein1 measured the rate at which the irritating process is transmitted along the muscle by recording the latent period, the time elapsing between the Fin. 34.— Method of recording the rate of passage of the contraction process along a muscle (after Marey). The movements of the muscle are recorded by means of air-transmission. The pince myo- graphique consists of two light bars, the upper of which acts as a lever, moving freely on an axis sup- ported by the lower. When the free end of the upper bar is raised, the other end presses down on a delicate rubber membrane which covers a little metal capsule, which is carried on the corresponding extremity of the Lower bar. The capsule is in air-communication, by a stiff-walled rubber tube, with another capsule which is similarly covered with rubber membrane. A light lever is connected with the membrane of the second tambour, and records its movements on the surface of a revolving cylinder. The muscle is placed between the free ends of the bars of the pince myographique, and, when the muscle thickens in contraction, it raises one end of the lever, depresses the membrane at the other end, and drives air into the recording tambour, and thus, by automatically raising the writing-point, records its change in form on the cylinder. moment of irritation and the beginning of the contraction (see p. 102). A lever was so connected with one end of the muscle as to record the instant that it began to thicken. The muscle was stimulated in one experiment at the end from which the record of its contraction was taken, and in another immediately following experiment it was stimulated near the other end. The distance between the stimulated points being known, the rate of transmission was reckoned from the difference in the latent periods. In his experiments he found the rate of conduction in the semimembranosus of the frog to be from 3.2 to 4.4 meters per second. Hermann found the rate to be 2.7 meters for the curarized sartorius of the frog. The results obtained by Abey and some others are a little lower, but probably 3 meters per second can be accepted as the average normal rate for frog's muscle. Length of Wave. — By such experiments it becomes obvious that the con- traction process passes over the muscle, in the form of a wave. In an experi- ment, such as Bernstein's, in which the thickening of the muscle is recorded, we can determine from the length of the curve written by the contracting muscle how long the contraction remains at a given place. Knowing this, and the rate at which the pine— passes along the fibre, we can estimate the length of the contraction wave, just as we could reckon the length of a train 1 Untersuchunyen iiber die elektrische Errerjung von Muskeln unci Nerven, 1871, S. 79. GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. 89 of cars if we knew how fast it was moving and how long; it required to pass a given station. Thus, if the contraction is found to last al a given point on the muscle 0.1 second, and the rate at which the contraction process is travelling is 3000 millimeters per second, the length of the wave is 300 milli- meters. According to Bernstein's determinations, the length of the wave of contraction in a frog's striated muscle is from 198-380 millimeters. The length of a striated muscle-fibre is, at the most, scarcely more than 40 milli- meters, and normally the muscle-fibre is stimulated, not as in the above ex- periment at one end, but near its centre, at the point where the nerve joins it; the irritation process spreads along the fibre in both directions from this point, and would pass over the distance 20 millimeters so quickly that practi- cally the whole muscle-fibre would be in the same phase of contraction at the same time. Rate of Conduction in Different Kinds of Muscle. — The rate of conduction varies very considerably in the muscles of different animals, and in different kinds of muscle in the same animal, just as the contraction process itself dif- fers in its rate and strength. Meters per second. Smooth muscle-fibres of the ureters of the rabbit . . . 0.02-0.03 (Engelmann). Muscle of the heart-ventricle of the frog 0.1 I Waller). Contractile substance of medusa; 0.5 (Waller). Neck-muscles of the turtle 0.1 -0.5 ( Hermann and Abey). Gracilis and semimembranosus of the frog .... 3.2 -4.4 (Bernstein). Cruralis (red muscle) of the rabbit 3.4 (Rollet). Sterno-mastoid of the dog 3. -6 (Bernstein and SteinerV Semimembranosus (white muscle) of the rabbit . . . 5.4-11.4 (Rollet). Human muscle 10. -13 (Hermann). (b) Rate of Conduction in Nerves. — Conductivity is most highly developed in the case of the nerve-fibre. The distances through which it acts and the rapidity of the process excite our wonder, 'flic process is accompanied by no visible change in the nerve-fibre itself, and the strength and rate have to be estimated by the effect produced on the organ which the nerve excites to action, or by the change which takes place in the electrical condition of the nerve as the wave of excitation sweeps over it. Rate in Motor Nerves. — Helmholtz was the first to measure the rate of con- duction in nerves.1 Originally he employed Pouillefs method for measuring short intervals of time. The arrangement is illustrated in Figure 35. The moment that a current was thrown into the coils of a galvanometer (see p. 145) the current in the primary coil of an induction apparatus was broken and the nerve connected with the secondary coil received a shork. An instant after the contraction of the muscle which resulted from the stimulation of the nerve broke the galvanometer circuit. The amount of deviation of the magnet of the galvanometer varied with the time that the circuit remained closed, and therefore could be taken as a measure of the interval elapsing between the stimulation of the nerve and the contraction of the muscle. The nerve was 1 Helmholtz: Archivfiir Anatomie und Physioloyu; 1850, S. 71-27U; 1852, S. 199. 90 AN AMERICAN TENT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. excited in two succeeding experiments at two points, at a known distance apart, and tin difference in the time records obtained was the time required for the transmission of the nerve-impulse through this distance. Fig. 35. — Method of estimating rate of conduction in motor nerve of frog, as vised by Helmholtz. The horizontal bara?> is supported on an axis in such a manner that when the contact is made at a it is broken at b, therefore at the same instant a current is made in the galvanometer circuit g and opened in tin- primary circuit of the induction apparatus /). When the muscle contracts, the galvanometer circuit is broken at c. The nerve was stimulated in two successive experiments at d and e. Later, Helmholtz devised a method by which a muscle would record its contractions on a rapidly moving surface, and employed this to measure the rate of conduction in motor nerves. He stimulated the nerve as near as possible to the muscle and let the contraction be recorded; then he stimulated the nerve as far as possible from the muscle, and again had the contraction recorded. The difference in time between the moment of excitation and the beginning of the contraction in the two experiments was due to the difference in the distance that the nerve-impulse had to pass in the two cases, and, this distance being known, the rate of conduction could be readily calculated. By this means he found the rate of transmission in the motor nerves of the frog to be 27 meters per second. In similar experiments upon men he recorded the contractions of the muscles of the ball of the thumb, and noted the difference in the time of the beginning of the contractions when the median nerve was excited through the skin at two different places. He found the average normal rate for man to be about 1)4 meters per second, a rate which is considerably quicker than that of our fastest express trains, but ;i million times less than the rate at which an electric current is trans- mitted along a wire. These determinations are still accepted as approxi- mately correct for human nerves, although they are found to vary very con- siderably under different conditions, a high temperature and strong irritation quickening the rate to 90 or more meters per second, while cooling may gradually -low the rate and finally stop conduction. Moreover, considerable differences exist in nerves controlling different functions, even in the same animal. Thus Chauveau gives the rate for the fibres of the vagus nerve, which supply the rapidly contracting striated muscles of the larynx, as 66.7 meters per second ; and the rate for vagus fibres, controlling the slower smooth muscles of the (esophagus, as 8.2 meters per second. The rate of transmission in the non-medullated nerves of invertebrates appears to be still GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. 91 slower; the nerve for the claw-muscles of the lobster conducts at a rate of from 6 to 12 meters per second, according as the temperature is low or high (Fredericq and Vandervelde). The rati' in non-medullated nerves of the Cephalopodia is 3.5-5.5 meters per second | Boruttau). Contrary to the view frequently expressed (Pfliiger1 and others), all parts of the nerve have the same rate of conduction.2 Rate in Sensory Ni roes. — We have no definite knowledge of the rate of conduction in sensory uerves. The attempt lias been made to measure it by stimulating the sensory fibres of a nerve-trunk at two different points and noting the difference in the time of the beginning of the resulting reflex acts ; or, in experiments on men, the difference in the length of the reaction time has been taken as an indication. By reaction time is meant the interval which elapses between the moment that the irritant is applied and the signal which is made by the subject as soon as he feels the sensation. Oehl found the mean normal rate of conduction in the sensory nerves of men to be 36.6 meters per second.3 Dolley and Cattell,4 by employing the reaction-time method, found the rate for the sensory fibres of the median nerve of one of them to be 21.1 meters per second, and for the other 49.5 meters per second, while the posterior tibial nerve gave rates, for one of them 31.2 meters, and for the other 64.9 meters. They attribute these wide variations in part to differences in the effectiveness of the irritant at different parts of the skin, but chiefly to differences in the activity of the central nervous processes involved in the act. Schelske ' observed similar differences in different men — for one 25.3 meters, for another 32.6 meters, and for still another 31.05 meters per second. In spite of the great difficulty of getting definite measurements in experi- ments on men, we may conclude from the work of these and other observers that the rate of conduction in sensory fibres is about the same as in motor fibres; in the case of man about 35 meters per second. Another method applicable to isolated nerves is based on the fact that the passage of the nerve-impulse along a nerve is accompanied by a change in its electrical condition. The rate of conduction can be ascertained by finding the rate at which this electrical change is transmitted. Influences which Alter the Rate and Strength of the Conduction-proc- ess.— (a) Effect of Death-processes. — Normally, the rate of conduction in mus- cle-fibres is SO rapid that the whole muscle appears to contracl at the same time ; but there are certain conditions under which the transmission of the exciting influence is very much slowed, <>r even altogether prevented, so that the stimu- lation of a given part of the muscle results in a local swelling, or welt, limited 1 Pfliiger: Uhtersuchungen itber die Physiologic da ElectrotoTvua, Berlin, 1859, S. 465. JR. du Rois-Reymon.l : CentralblcUtfur Physiologic, 1899, Bd. xiii. S. 513. 3()ehl : Archives italiennes de Biologic, 189"), xxi. .'>, p. 401. *Dollc-y and ('attell : Psychological Review, New York and London, 1 894, i. p. 159. 'Schelske: Archiv fur Anatomic uml Plu/sinlniiir, lStil, S. 151 . 92 AN AMERICAN TENT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. to the excited area. When a muscle is dying, the rate of conduction as well as the rapidity of contraction is lessened. The muscles of warm-blooded ani- mals exhibit more striking differences than those of cold-blooded, but both are similarly affected. 1 fa dying muscle be mechanically stimulated, as by a direct blow, a localized swelling develops at the place; and if the muscle be stroked with a dull instrument, a wave of contraction maybe seen to follow the instru- ment, the contraction being quite strictly limited to the excited area, so that one can write on the muscle. The strict localization of the contraction to the irritated parts makes it evident that the nerves take no part in it, hence Schiff called it an idio-muscular contraction, in distinction from the normal neuro- muscular contraction. In the dying nerve as in the dying muscle the rate of transmission is found to be slowed. (6) Effect of Mechanical Conditions. — The effect of pressure to lessen the conduction-power of nerves is one which everyone has had occasion to demon- strate on himself. For example, if pressure be brought to bear on the ulnar nerve where it crosses the elbow, the region supplied by the nerve becomes numb, "goes to sleep," so to speak. It is noticeable that only a slightly greater effort is required to move the muscles, at a time when no sensations are received from the hand. For some unexplained reason the sensory nerve-fibres appear to be less resistant than the motor. Gradually applied pressure may paralyze the nerve without exciting it, but on the removal of the pressure the recovery of function of the sensory fibres is accompanied by excitation processes, which are felt as pricking sensations referred to the region supplied by the nerve. The exact reason of the loss of functional power caused by pressure which is insuf- ficient to produce permanent injury is not altogether clear. Stretching a nerve may act to lessen, and if severe destroy, conductivity. It is in one sense another way of applying pressure, since the calibre of the sheath is lessened and through the fluids pressure is brought to bear on the axis-cylinder. Of course, if the stretching were excessive, the nerve-fibres would be ruptured and degenerate. Whether stretching can alter the rate of conduction in nerves is not known. Apparently it does not do so in muscles, although because of the greater length of the muscle it appears to do so.1 (c) Effect of Temperature on Conduction. — Helmholtz and Baxt found that by cooling motor nerves they could lower the rate of conduction, and by heat- ing them they could increase it even more markedly. By altering the tem- perature of the motor nerves of man, they observed rates varying from 30 to 90 meters per second. The rate of the motor nerves of other animals is like- wise greatly altered by heat and cold. This is true of the invertebrates as well ;i~ the vertebrates; the rate in the nerves of the claw-muscles of the lobster, for example, changes from 6 to 12 meters per second as the temperature is varied from 10° to 20° C. Sensory nerve-fibres are similarly influenced by temperature. Oehl 2 found by cooling and heating the nerves of men, varia- tions of from 30 to 25 meters per second on cooling, and from 30 to 50 meters 1 Schenck : Pfiuger's Archiv, 1896, Bd. 64, S. 179. 'Oehl: Archives italiennes de Bioloyie, 1895, xiiv. p. 231. GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. 93 a second on heating. Both the sympathetic and the vagus nerve-fibres in the frog have their influence on the heart-beat decreased by cold and increased by heat.1 The Favorable influence of heat on the conduction power seems common to all nerves, but only within certain limits. The motor fibres of the sciatic of the frog lose their power to conduct at 41° to 44° C, but may recover the power if quickly cooled; if the temperature lias readied 50° C. conductivity is per- manently lost. Nerves of like function in different animals may lose the power of conduc- tion at different temperatures. Thus the motor fibres of the sciatic nerve of the dog cease to conduct at 6° C, those of the cat at 5° to 3° C, of the frog at about 0° C. The inhibitory fibres of the vagus nerve of the dog show dimin- ished activity at 3° C, and become wholly inactive at 0° C. ; the inhibitory fibres of the vagus of the rabbit become inactive at 15° ( '. Different kinds of fibres in the same nerve-trunk may be differently affected by temperature, and this difference may be sufficiently marked in some cases to be used as a means of distinguishing them.2 For example, the temperature limits at which the vaso-constrictor fibres of the sciatic of the cat can conduct are 2°-3° C. to 47° C, while the limits for the dilator fibres are both lower and higher than for the constrictors. If cold be applied to the sciatic nerve, the fibres supplying the extensor muscles seem to fail before those which in- nervate the flexors. Further, it has been observed that if cold be applied locally to a nerve, the part affected loses its power to conduct, and acts as a block to the passage of the nerve-impulse generated in another part of the nerve. Application of extreme cold to the ulnar nerve of man at the elbow results in a complete loss of feeling in the parts which the nerve supplies.3 On the other hand, the strength of an impulse is increased by passage through a region which has been warmed. These facts remind us of the effect of heat and cold <>n the activity of other forms of protoplasm and would find a comparatively easy explanation were we content to look upon conduction as the result of chemical change in the axis-cylinder. The fact that conduction does not cause fatigue is opposed to such an explanation, and so we take refuge in the idea that heat is favorable and cold unfavorable to molecular activity in general. (d) Effect of Chemicals . p. 22. 'Howell, Budgett, and Leonard i Journal <;/' Physiology, vol. xvi. Nob. •" ami t, L894. 3 Weir Mitchell : Injuries of Nerves and their Consequences, Philadelphia, 1S72, p. 59. * Griinhagen : PflUger'a Archiv, 1872, vi. 8. 180. 94 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. ductivity is dependent on other properties of the nerve than irritability, and there are some other facts pointing in the same direction ; for example, regenerating nerves acquire the power to conduct before they recover their irritability. The usual explanation of those who regard conduction as due to the excitation of each succeeding part of the nerve by the one just pre- ceding it is, that external excitation is a coarse affair as compared with the normal internal excitation process, and the effect of the former may be lost when the latter is still effective. (e) Effect of a Constant Battery Current. — A constant electric current, if allowed to flow through a nerve or muscle, not only alters the irritability, but also the conductivity. The change in the conductivity affects both the strength and rate of the conduction process. Yon Bezold1 found that weak and medium currents have little effect on the conductivity, but that strong currents completely destroy the power of the nerve to transmit the nerve-impulse. As the strength of the current is increased, the first effect is observed at the anode, and shows itself in a slowing of the passage of the exciting impulse. This action is the greater the more of the nerve exposed to the current, the stronger the current, and the longer it is closed. The loss of conduction power is asso- ciated with changes at the place where the current enters and where it leaves the nerve rather than with alterations within the intrapolar region. Engelmann, in his experiments on the smooth muscle-fibres of the ureter, saw a decline of power of conduction at the anode by weak currents, which when the strength of the current was increased appeared also at the kathode; the conductivity was wholly lost at both poles when the current was very strong. In the case of a striated muscle, such as the sartorius of the frog, the kathode has been found to become impassable after strong currents have flowed through a muscle for a considerable time. The same is true of nerves. It is not surprising that a current which can greatly decrease the irritability at the anode, and even inhibit a contraction which may be present when it is applied, should be found to decrease the conductivity as well, but that the con- ductivity should be decreased at the kathode, where the irritability is greatly increased, was not to be expected. Rutherford -found that with weak currents the rate of the conduction power at the kathode was increased rather than diminished, and that it was only when strong currents acted a considerable time that the conduction power lessened at the kathode. Biedermann explains this on the ground that the increased excitability at the kathode leads in the muscle to a condition of latent contraction and therefore to fatigue, and that it is this which lessens the conductivity. The lessened power to conduct con- tinues at the kathode alter the removal of the current. There is little doubt that fatigue interferes with the conduction power of muscle, but this explana- tion would hardly apply to nerves which are not known to fatigue at the point of stimulation, i. e. if we limit the term fatigue to changes resulting from physiological activity. Undoubtedly chemical and physical alterations may 1 Unterauehungen iiber die elektrische Erregung der Nerval und Muskeln, Leipzig, 1861. 'Journal of Anatomy and Physiology, 1867, vol. 2, p. 87. GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. 95 occur in nerves as a result of the passage of an electric current through them, and it would seem as if the loss of conductivity which they show when sub- jected to strong currents is to be accounted for by such electrolytic changes. The changes produced in the conductivity of nerves by Btrong currents explain the failure of the closing of the ascending current and opening of the descending current t<> irritate the muscle (see Pfluger's law, p. 50). In the former case the anode regit I" decreased conductivity intervenes between the kathode, where the closing stimulus is developed, and the muscle. In the latter case the irritation developed at the anode, on the opening of the current, is unable to pass the region of decreased conductivity which is formed at the kathode, and which persists after the current is opened. Practical Application <>J Alterations produced by Battery Currents. — The alterations produced by strong battery currents in the irritability and conduc- tivity of nerves and muscles may be made use of by the physician. If the effect of only one pole is desired, it may be applied as a small electrode im- mediately over the region to lie influenced, while the other pole may be a large electrode placed over some distant part of the body where there are no import- ant organs. The size of the electrodes used determines the density of the current leaving or entering the body and consequently the intensity of its action. The application of the anode to a region of increased excitability, by decreasing the irritability, may for the time lessen irritation; on the other hand the kathode may heighten the irritability of a region of decreased excitability. The sending of a strong polarizing current through a motor nerve, by lessening the conductivity, may prevent abnormal motor impulses from reaching muscles, and so stop harmful "cramps;" or the sending of such a current through a sensory nerve may, during the (low of the cur- rent, keep painful impulses from reaching the central nervous system. In applying a strong battery current to lessen irritability or conductivity it must be remembered that the after-effect of such a current is increased irritability. (/) Efect of Conduction and Fatigue of Nerves. — Many experiments have been made in the hope of detecting sonic form of chemical change as a result of conduction. The nerve has been stimulated for many hours in succession with an electric current, and then been examined with the utmost care to find whether there had been an accumulation of some waste product, as carbon dioxide, or some other acid body. The gray matter of the spinal cord, which is largely composed of nerve-cells, is found to become acid a- a result of activity,1 but this cannot be found to be the case with the white matter of the cord, which is chiefly made up of nerve-fibres, nor has an acid reaction been obtained with certainty in nerve-trunks. - 1Funke: Archivfur AruUomieund l'liiisi'>l<><)i<\ 1 s ">'.>, S. 835. Ranker Cnitnilblutt fur medicin- ische Wixseiisrlmfl, ISfiS and lNfif). 2 Heidenliain : Studien aus dem phyaiologuchen Tnalitui ra Bresiau, ix. S. 248 ; Centralblatt fur Medicin, 1868, S. 833. Ti.ijersiedt : "Studien iilier iiieclianiselie Nervenreizuni;," Ada Societatia Scientiamm Fennicce, 1880, torn. xi. 96 AN AMERICAN TEXT- BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. Not only has an attempt to discover this or other waste products which might be supposed to result from chemical changes within the nerve-fibre failed, but observers have been unable to obtain evidence of the liberation of heat, which one would expect to find were the nerve-fibre the seat of chem- ical changes during the process of conduction.1 Stewart writes: "Speaking quite roughly, I think we may say that in the nerves of rabbits and dogs there is not even a rise of temperature of the general nerve-sheath of 2Q100 of a degree during excitation." .Many experiments have been made to ascertain whether a nerve would fatigue if made to conduct for a long; time. Most of these have been made upon motor nerves, the amount of contraction of the muscle, in response to a definite stimulus applied to the nerve, being taken as an index of the activity of the nerve. Since the muscle would fatigue if stimulated continuously for a long time, various means have been employed to block the nerve-impulse and prevent it from reaching the muscle, except at the beginning and end of the experiment. This block has been established by passing a continuous current through the nerve near the muscle, thus inducing an electrotonic change and non-conducting area;2 or the nerve-ends were poisoned with curare (see p. 26), and the nerve excited until the effect of the drug wore off, and the nerve-impulse was able to reach the muscle '/ or the part of the nerve near the muscle was temporarily deprived of its conducting power by an anaesthetic, such as ether. Another method of experimentation consisted in using the negative variation current of a nerve (see p. 150) as an indication of its activity, the presence of the current being observed with the galvanom- eter.* Other experimenters have examined the vagus nerve, to see if after long-continued stimulation it was still capable of inhibiting the heart, the effect of the stimulation being prevented from acting on the heart muscle during the experiment by atropin,5 or by cold, applied locally to the nerve.6 Still another method was to study the effect of long-continued stimulation on the secretory fibres of the chorda tympani, the exciting impulse being kept from the gland-cells by atropin.7 Most of these experiments have yielded nega- tive results, and it is doubtful whether nerves are fatigued by the process of conduction. These results, of course, do not show that the nerve-fibres can live and function independently of chemical changes. As has been said, nerves lose their irritability in time if deprived of the normal blood-supply, and undoubtedly they arc, like all protoplasmic structures, continually the seat of metabolic 1 Helmholtz: Archiv fiir Analomie und Physiologie, 1848, S. 158. Heidenhain : op. cit. Rolleston: Journal of Physiology, 1890, vol. xi. p. 208. Stewart: ibid., 1891, vol. xii. p. 424. 2 Bernstein : Pfiiiger's Archiv, 1877, xv. S. 289. Wedenski : Centralblatt fur die medic inischen Wwaenschqften, 1884. •, Bowditch : Journal of Physiology, 1885, vi. p. 133. * Wedenski : loc. cit. Maschek : Sittungsberichte der Wiener Academic, 1887, Bd. xcv. Abthl. 3. 5 Szana: Archiv fur Anatomic und Physiologie, 1891, 8. 315. 6 Howell, Budgett, and Leonard: Journal of Physiology, 1894, xvi. p. 312. 7 Lambert: Comples-rendus dc la Societe de Biologie, 1894, p. 511. GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NEHVE. 97 processes. The normal function of the nerve, however, the conduction of the nerve-impulse, seems to take place without any marked chemical change. Nature of the Conduction Process. — There have been a great many views as to the nature of the conduction process, one after the other being advanced and combated as physiological facts bearing on the question have been accumulated. It has been suggested that the whole nerve moved like a bell-rope; that the nerve was a tube, and that a biting acid flowed along it ; that the nerve contained an elastic fluid which was thrown into oscillations; that it conducted an electric current, like a wire; that it was composed of definitely arranged electro-motor molecules which exerted an electro-dynamic effect on each other; that it was made up of chemical particles, which like the particles of powder in a fuse, underwent an explosive change, each in turn exciting its neighbor; that the irritant caused a chemical change, which produced an alteration of the electrical condition of such a nature as to excite neighboring parts to chemical change and thereby to electrical change, and so alternating chemical and electrical changes progressed along the fibre in the form of a wave; finally, that the molecules of the nerve-substance underwent a form of physical vibration analogous to that assumed lor light. A discussion of these different theories, none of which can be regarded as entirely satisfactory, cannot be entered upon here. Although the exact nature of the conduction process is not determined, there seems little doubt that it is of the same type in all forms of protoplasm. In all cases it is a property of the living substance of the cell and is lost when the cell dies : the state of activity spreads like a wave in all directions through the living substance, and is markedly altered by physical and chem- ical influences which change the irritability of the living substance, and in much the same way as this is altered ; continuity of protoplasm is absolutely essential to conduction, hence the spread of the excitation change is limited to the one cell, unless the cell is connected by protoplasmic bridges with other cells, or possesses a specially differentiated exciting end-organ. In its details the conduction process exhibits many peculiarities in differ- ent cells and even in the different parts of* the same cell. The receiving organs at the extremities of the dendrites of different classes of neurones differ widely in respect to structure, and in their capacity t<. react to different kinds of stimuli and to transmit the state of excitation to the dendrite. The exciting organs at the extremities of the axonee of different .'lasses of neurones are of different types, and behave differently, the discharge of the exciting process upon a muscle, gland, or nerve-cell being adjusted to the capacity for reaction possessed by the organ in question. In each neurone the strands of protoplasm which connect these distant receiving and exciting mechanisms with the cell body, and the body of the cell itself, work each according to its own nature. For example, the time spent by the phase of activity in tin- body of a ganglion-cell of the posterior Bpinal root-ganglion, is far longer than that used in a corresponding length of protoplasm in the dendrite of the cell. Although the conduction process differs in its details even in different Vol. II.— 7 98 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. parts of the same neurone, the condition of activity which spreads through the neurone and which we call the nerve impulse, has the same general character- istics in all forms of nerves whether medullated or non-medullated, motor, sensory, or secretory. The character of a movement or secretion depends on the character of the organ excited, and not on the nature of the change trans- mitted along the efferent nerve, and the specific character of a special sensa- tion depends on the form of psychic activity developed in the central nervous system, and not on the nature of the process of transmission in the afferent neurone. This view that the nerve impulse is to be regarded merely as an excitatory process, and that it has the same general characteristics in all kinds of nerves, is strengthened by two sets of experiments which have been reported lately. One of these sets of experiments was reported by Langley.1 He found that preganglionic sympathetic fibres — i.e. fibres between the ganglion and the cord — if cut centrally from the ganglion, after a time regenerate and make new connections with the nerve-cells of the ganglion. In some cases they unite with cells of their own class, and sometimes with other cells; for example, pupil lo-dilator fibres were found to have established connection with pilo-motor neurones — i. e. with ganglion-cells which send their axones to the erector muscles of the hairs. Further, by section of post-gangl ionic fibres — i. e. fibres between the ganglion and the periphery — it was found, after regeneration had occurred, that pilo-motor fibres can form nerve-endings in the iris and become pupillo-dilator fibres. Evidently ganglion-cells and muscle-fibres can be excited by nerve impulses developed in other nerves than those normally connected with them. A still more remarkable result was obtained by Budgett and Green. A description of this experiment is given on page 85. They succeeded in causing sensory fibres of the pneumogastric to grow down a degenerated motor trunk, the hypoglossal, and connect with the muscles of the tongue. In this case excitation of the peripheral part of the afferent nerve caused muscular contractions. If we should think of the nerve which was excited, we would be inclined to say that a sensory impulse was generated ; if we should think of the effect on the muscle, we would call it a motor impulse, and the latter would be the proper term. Evidently the condition of activity which can be aroused in a sensory nerve is capable under suitable conditions of exciting muscles, and sensory nerves cannot be considered to be the seat of specific forms of energy different from those generated in motor nerves. D. Contractility. Contractility is the property of protoplasm by virtue of which the cell is able to change its form when subjected to certain external influences called irritants, or when excited by certain changes occurring within itself. The change of form does not involve a change of size. The contraction is the 1 Langley : Journal of Physiology, 1897, xxii. p. 215. GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. 99 result of a change in the position of the more fluid parts of the cell-protoplasm, and the effect is to cause the cell to approach a spherical shape. In the case of an amoeba, for instance, excitation causes a drawing in of the pseudopods, and as the material in them flows back into the cell the body of the cell expands and acquires a globular form. In the simpler forms of contractile protoplasm the movement does not appear to be limited to any special direction, but in the case of the highly differentiated forms, such as muscle, both contraction and relaxation occur on definite lines. When a muscle is excited to action, energy is liberated through chemical change of certain constituents of the muscle-substance, and this energy in some unknown way causes a rearrangement of the finest particles of the muscle-sub- stance, and the consequent change of form peculiar to the contracted state. When the irritation ceases and relaxation takes place, there is a sudden return of the muscle-substance to the position of rest, either because of elastic recoil or of some other force at work within the muscle itself. That the recovery of the elongated form peculiar to the resting muscle is not dependent on external influences is evidenced by the fact that a muscle floating on mercury, and subjected to no extending force, will on the cessation of irritation assume its resting form. The relaxation no less than the contraction must be regarded as an active process, but on account of their flexibility muscle-fibres are incap- able of exerting an expansion force, therefore cannot by relaxing do external work. Both the histological structure and physiological action of the striated mus- cles which move the bones show them to be the most highly differentiated, the most perfect form of contractile tissue. It is by means of these structures that the higher animals perform all those voluntary movements by which they change their position with reference to external objects, acquire nourishment, protect themselves, and influence their surroundings. An exact knowledge of the method of action of these mechanisms and the influences which affecl them is therefore of the greatest importance to us. 1. Simple Muscle -Contractions Studied by the Graphic Method. — When a muscle makes a single contraction, in response to an electric shock or other irritant, the change of form is too rapid to be followed by the eye. To acquire an adequate idea of the character of the movement it is necessary that we should obtain a continuous record of the alterations in shape which it un- dergoes. This can be done by connecting the muscle with a mechanism which enables it automatically to record its movements. If one moves a pencil vertically up and down on a piece of paper, a straight line is written ; it' while the vertical movements are continued the paper be drawn along at a regular rate in a direction at right angles to the move- ment of the pencil, a curve will be traced. If the paper be moved at a regular rate, the shape of the curve will depend on the rate at which the pencil is moved, and, if the speed of the paper be known, the rate of movement of the pencil can be readily determined. This principle is employed in recording the movements of muscles. The muscle is connected with a mechanism which 100 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. rises and falls as the muscle contracts and relaxes, and records the movement of the muscle on a surface which passes by the writing-point at a regular speed (see Fig. 38); such a record is called a myogram. The Myograph. — The writing mechanism, together with the apparatus which moves the surface on which the record of the movement of a contracting muscle is taken is called a myograph. The writing mechanism has usually the form of a light, stiff lever, which moves very easily on a delicate axis; the lever is so connected with the muscle as to magnify its movements. The point of the lever rests very lightly against a glass plate, or surface covered with glazed paper, which is coated with a thin layer of soot. The point of the lever scratches off the soot, and the movements are recorded as a very fine white line. At the close of the experiment the record is made permanent by passing it through a thin alcoholic solution of shellac. The recording surface in some cases is in the form of a plate, in others of a cyl- inder, and is moved at a regular rate by a spring, pendu- lum, falling weight, clockwork, electric or other motor.1 The record which is traced with the myograph lever by the muscle has the form of a curve. From the height of the curve we can readily estimate the amount that the muscle changes its length, but in order to accu- rately determine the duration of the contraction process and the time relations of different parts of the curve, it is necessary to know the exact rate at which the recording surface is moving. The shape of the curve drawn by the muscle will depend very largely on the rate of the movement of the surface on which the record is taken. This is illustrated by the four records repro- duced in Figure 36. These were all taken from the same muscle within a few minutes of each other and under exactly the same conditions, except that in the successive experiments the speed of the drum on which the record was traced was increased. A udauce at these records shows that a knowledge of the rate of movement of the surface on which the record is taken is indis- pensable to an understanding of the time relations of the different parts of the curve written by the muscle. The rate of movement of the recording surface can !><• registered by an instrument called a chronograph. The chronograph (g, Fig. 37), consists of one or two coils of wire wound round cores of soft iron, and a little lever bearing a strip of iron, which is attracted to the soft-iron cores whenever they are magnetized by an elec- tric current flowing through the coils of wire about them. When the current ceases to flow and the iron ceases to be magnetized, a spring draws the lever 1 See O. Langendorfl': Physiologuehe Graphik, Kranz Deuticke, Leipzig, 1891 ; J. S. Brodie : The Essentials of Experimental Physiology, London, 1898. Fig. 36.— Records of four contractions of a gas- trocnemius muscle of a frog: a, recording sur- faee at nst; b, surface moving slowly; c, sur- face moving more rapidly ; '/, surface moving even faster. GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. 101 away from the iron. Many of the instruments employed for this purpose are very delicate, and are capable of responding to very rapid interruptions of the Fig. 37.— Method of interrupting an electric circuit by a tuning-fork, and of recording the Interrup- tions by means of an electro-magnet: a, battery; b, tuning-fork, with platinum wire at the extremity of one of its arms, which with each vibration of the fork makes and breaks contact with the mercury in the cup below; c, mercury cup; e, electro-magnet which keeps the fort vibrating; g, chronograph. The current from the battery a, passes to the fork b, then, by way of the platinum wire, to the mercury in cup c, then to the binding-post d, where it divides, a part going through the coils of wire of the chronograph g, and thence to the binding-post/, the rest through the coil of wire of electro-magnet e, and then to the post/, from which the united threads of current flow back to the battery. The electro-magnet e keeps the fork in vibration, because when the platinum wire enters the mercury at c, the circuit is completed and the electro-magnet magnetizes its Boft-iron core, which attracts the arms of the fork, and thus draws the wire out of the mercury and so breaks the circuit. When the current is broken the fork, being released, springs back, dips the wire into the mercury, and by closing the circuit causes the process to be repeated. current. The electric current is made and broken at regular intervals by a clock. tuning-fork (b, Fig. 37), or other interrupting mechanism, and the lever of the chronograph, which has a writing-point at its free end, moves correspondingly Fig. 38.— Myogram from gastrocnemius muscle of frog ; beneath, the time is recorded in 0.005 second ; a, moment of excitation ; t>, beginning of contraction ; <\ height of contraction ; '/, end of contraction. and traces an interrupted line on the recording surface of the myograph (see Fig. 38). The space between the succeeding jogs marked by the chronograph lever is a measure of the amount of the surface which passed the point of the chronograph in one second, J-,, second, or , fo second, as the case may be. Myogram of Simple Muscle-contraction. — The rate of the movement of the muscle during every pari of its contraction can be n'adilv determined by com- paring the record it lias drawn with that of the chronograph. Figure 38 is the reproduction of a single contraction of a gastrocnemius muscle of a frog. The rise of the curve shows that the contraction began comparatively slowly. Boon became very rapid, but toward its close was again gradual.; the relaxation began almost immediately, and took a similar course, 102 AX AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. though occupying a somewhat longer time. The electric current which actuated the chronograph was made and broken by a tuning-fork which made 200 complete vibrations per second, therefore the spaces between the succeeding peaks of the chronograph curve each represents 0.005 second. A comparison of the movements of the muscle with the tuning-fork curve reveals that about jfo second elapsed between the point b, at which the muscle curve began to rise, and c, the point at which the full height of the contraction was reached, and that about y^-g- second was occupied by the return of the muscle curve from c to point d, at the level from which it started. The muscle employed in this experiment was slightly fatigued, and the movements were in consequence a little slower than normal. Latent Period. — The time that elapses between the moment that a stim- ulus reaches a muscle and the instant the muscle begins to change its form is called the latent period. In the experiment recorded in Fig. 38 the muscle received the shock at the point a on the curve, but the lever did not begin to rise until the point b was reached. The latent period as recorded in this ex- periment was about 0.006 second. The latent period and the time relations of the muscle-curve were first measured by Helmholtz, who introduced the use of the myograph.1 Helmholtz concluded from his experiments that the latent period for a frog's muscle is about y^ second, that the rise of the curve occupies about jfo, and the fall about j^-g- second, the total time occupying about -^ second. These rates can be considered approximately correct, excepting for the latent period, which has been found by more accurate methods to be con- siderably shorter. Tigerstedt connected a curarized frog's muscle with a myo- graph lever, which was so arranged as to break an electric contact at the instant that the muscle made the slightest movement ; the break in the electric circuit was recorded on a rapidly revolving drum, by an electro-magnet similar to the chronograph. By this means he found the latent period of a frog's muscle may be as short as 0.004 second. Tigerstedt2 did not regard this as the true latent period, however; he expressed the belief that the muscle proto- plasm must have begun to respond to the excitation much sooner than this. The contraction of the whole muscle is the result of a shortening of each of the myriad of light and dark disks of which each of the muscle-fibres is composed (see Fig. 39). The distance to be traversed by the finest particles of muscle- substance is microscopic, hence the rapidity of the change of form of the whole muscle. Even such a change would require time, however, and it is probable that the muscle protoplasm becomes active before any outward manifestation occurs. That this view is correct has been proved by electrical observations. When muscle protoplasm passes from a state of rest to one of action it undergoes an alteration in electrical condition. This change can be detected by the galvanometer (Fig. 62, p. 144) or by the capillary electrometer (Fig. 63, p. 146). Burdon Sanderson s has found that by the aid of the latter instru- 1 Archiv fur Anatcmiie und Physiologie, 1850, S. 308. 7 Ibid., 1885, Suppl. Bd., S. 111. 6 Journal of Physiology, 1898, vol. xxiii. p. 350. GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. 103 ment an alteration of the electrical condition of the muscle of a frog can be detected less than 0.001 second after the stimulus has been applied to it. Since some slight interval of time must have been lost even by this delicate method, it Mould seem that muscle protoplasm begins to be active at the instant it is stimulated. According to this view, muscle-substance has no latent period ; neverthe- less we can still speak of the latent period of the muscle as a whole. It will be necessary, however, to distinguish between the electrical latent period and the mechanical latent period : by the former we mean the time which elapses between the moment of excitation and the first evidence obtainable of a change in the electrical condition of the muscle ; by the latter, the time between exci- tation and the earliest evidence of movement which cau be observed. In the case of the striated muscles of a frog the electrical latent period is less than 0.001 second, and the mechanical about 0.004 second. Mendelssohn ' estimated the mechanical latent period of the muscles of man to be about 0.008 second. There can be little doubt, however, that this figure is too large. Bernstein2 found that if a normal frog's muscle be excited indirectly, by the stimulation of its nerve, the mechanical latent period is somewhat longer than when it is directly excited. Of course a certain length of time is required to transmit the excitation through the length of nerve intervening between the point stimulated and the muscle fibres. If this time be deducted, there still remains a balance of about 0.003 second, which can only be ac- counted for on the assumption that the motor nerve end-plates require time to excite the muscle-fibres. The motor end-plates are therefore said to have a latent period of 0.002-0.003 second. The latent period, and the time required for the rise and fall of the myo- graph curve, are found to be very different not only for the muscles of differ- ent animals, but even for the different muscles of the same animal. Moreover, the time relations of the contraction process in each muscle are altered by a great variety of conditions. Before considering the effect of various influences upon the character of the muscle contraction, let us give a glance at the finer structure of the muscle, and the change of form which the microscopic segments of the muscle-fibre undergo during contraction. 2. Optical Properties of Striated Muscle during Rest and Action. — An ordinary striated muscle is composed of a great number of very long muscle-cells, fibres as they are called, arranged side by side in bundles, the whole being bound together by a fine connective-tissue network. Each muscle- fibre consists (if a very delicate elastic sheath, the sarcoleimna, which is com- pletely filled with the muscle-substance. Under the microscope the fibres are seen to be striped by alternating light and dark transverse bands, and on focus- ing, the difference in texture which this suggests is found to extend through 1 Archives de Physiologic, 1880, 2d series, t. vii. p. 197. 2 Untersuchungen iiber den Erregungsvorgang im Nen>en und Muxkelsystem, 1871. 104 AJV AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY the fibres, i. e. the light and dark bands correspond to little disks of substances of different degrees of translucency. More careful study with a high power, shows under certain circumstances other cross markings (see Fig. 39, A), the light band is found to be divided in halves by a fine dark line, Z, and parallel to it is another faint dark line, n, while the dark band, Q, is found to have a barely per- ceptible light line in its centre. The fine dark lines, Z, which run through the middle of the light bands, Mere for a time supposed to be caused by delicate membranes (Krause's membrane), which were thought to stretch through the fibre and to divide it into a series of ittle compartments, each of which had exactly the same construction. Kuehne fig. 39.-Sehema of histological structure of chancecl to see a minute nematode worm muscle-fibre: A, resting fibre as seen by ordinary light; b, resting fibre seen by polarized light; c, moving along inside a muscle-fibre, and contracting fibre by ordinary light; 1>, contract- i -i ,1 , • , , 1 1 , big fibre by polarized light ' observed that it encountered no obstruc- tion, such as a series of membranes, how- ever delicate, would have caused. As it moved, the particles of muscle-sub- stance closed in behind it, the original structure being completely recovered. This observation did away with the view that the fibre is divided into com- partments, but the arrangement shown in Figure 39, A, repeats itself through- out the length of the fibre and indicates that it is made up of a vast succession of like parts. Muscle-substance consists of two materials, which differ in their optical peculiarities and their reaction to stains. If a muscle-fibre be examined by polarized light, it is found that there is a substance in the dark bands which refracts the light doubly, is anisotropic, while the bulk of the substance in the light bands is singly refractive, isotropic (Z>, Fig. 39). The anisotropic sub- stance is found to stain with hematoxylin, while the isotropic is not thus stained ; on the other hand, the isotropic substance is often colored by chloride of gold, which is not the case with the anisotropic. By means of these reac- tions it has been possible to ascertain something as to the arrangement of these substances within the muscle-fibre, though the ultimate structure has not been definitely decided. It appears that the isotropic material is the sarcoplasma, which is distributed throughout the fibre and holds imbedded within it the particles of the anisotropic substance, these particles having a definite arrange- ment. Striated muscle-fibres present not only cross markings, but under favorable conditions longitudinal striations, these being most evident in the dark bands. These longitudinal striations are looked upon with great interest as indicating that the particles of anisotropic material are arranged in long chains as incomplete fibrillar According to this view the muscle-fibre is com- GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. 105 posed of semifluid isotropic substance, in which are the particles of anisotropic material, arranged to form vast numbers of parallel fibrillar of like structure, and so placed as to give the effect of transverse disks (Z, n, (J, Fig. 39). When a striated muscle contracts, each of its fibres becomes shorter and thicker, and the same is true of the dark and light disks of which the fibres are composed. If we examine a muscle-fibre which has been fixed by osmic acid at a time when part of it was contracting, we see that in the contracted part the light and dark bands have both become shorter and wider, but that the volume of the dark bands (Q, Fig. 39, ( ') has increased at the expense of the light bands. Further, the dark bands are seen to be lighter and the light bands darker in the contracted part, while examination with polarized light shows that though the anisotropic substance does not seem to have changed its position, (Fig. 39, D), the original dark bands have less and the lighter bands greater refractive power. These appearances would seem to be explained by Engel- mann's view that contraction is the result of imbibition of the more fluid part of the sareoplasm by the anisotropic substance. He has advanced the theory that the cause of the imbibition is the liberation of heat by chemical changes which occur at the instant the muscle is excited. In support of this theory Engelmann ' showed that dead substance containing anisotropic material, such as a catgut string, can change its form, by imbibition of fluid under the influence of heat, and give a contraction curve in many respects similar to that to be obtained from muscle. This theory of the method of action of the muscle-substance, though attractive, can be accepted only as a working hypothesis, and is not to be regarded as proved. Various other theories have been advanced to explain the connection between the chemical changes which undoubtedly occur during contraction and the alteration of form, but none have been generally accepted. Enough has been said to show that the contraction of the muscle a- a whole is the result of a change in the minute elements of the fibrillffi, and that the various condi- tions which influence the activity of the process of contraction musl act chiefly through alterations produced in these little mechanisms. 3. Elasticity of Muscle. — The elasticity and extensibility of muscle are of great importance, for by every form of muscular work the muscle is sub- jected to a stretching force. Elasticity of muscle i- the property by virtue of which it tends to preserve its normal form, and to resist any external force which would act to alter that form. The shape of muscles may he altered by pressure, but the change is one of form and not of bulk ; since muscles arc largely made up of fluid, their compressibility is i asiderable. The elasticity of muscles is slight but quite perfect, by which is meant that a muscle yields readily to a Btretching force, hut on the removal of the force quickly recovers its normal form. Most of the experiments Upon muscle elasticity have been made after the muscle had been removed from the body, hence under abnormal 1 fiber den Ursprung a suspended muscle, one after the other, the extension pro- duced is not, like that of an inorganic body such as steel spring, proportional to the weight, but each weight stretches the muscle less than the preceding. If the weights be removed in succession, an elastic recovery is observed, which, although considerable, is incomplete. If the change in the length be recorded by a lever attached to the muscle, the surface being moved along just the same amount after each weight is added or removed, a curve is obtained such as is shown in Fig. 40, b. Above this is a record taken in a similar way from a piece of rubber (a). The rubber resem- bles a steel spring in that equal weights stretch it to like amounts, but the elastic recovery, though more complete than that of the muscle, is imperfect. In such an experiment it is found that the full eifect of adding the weights, or removing them from the muscle, does not occur immedi- ately, but when a weight is added there is a gradual yielding to the stretching force, and, on the removal of a weight, a gradual recovery of form under the influence of the elasticity. This slow after-action makes it difficult to say just what is to be considered the proper curve of elasticity of muscle, especially as the physiological condition of the muscle is always changing. The elasticity of muscles is dependent on normal physiological conditions, and is altered by death, or by anything which causes a change in the normal constitution of the muscles, as the cutting off of the blood-supply. The dead muscle is less extensible and less elastic than the normal living muscle. Heating, within limits, increases, and cooling decreases the elasticity, possibly by altering the mobility of the semifluid materials of the muscle, and hence changing the internal friction.1 Contraction is accom- panied by increased extensibility, i. e. lessened elasticity — and the changes caused by fatigue lessen the elasticity. It is interesting to note in this con- nection that the elasticity is decreased by weak acid solutions and increased by weak alkaline solutions (Brunton and Cash).2 The elasticity of a muscle within the body is generally considered to be more perfect than that of the isolated muscle, but even here one can observe the after-stretching described by Weber and the contraction remainder described by Hermann. Mosso3 suggests the following experiment on man : 1 Blix : Skandinavisches ArchivJ'ur Physiologie, 1893, iv. S. 392. 2 Philosophical Transactions, 1884, p. 197. s Mosso : Archives ilaliennes de Biologie, 1895, xxv. p. 27. GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. 107 Place the subject in a sitting position, make the suspended leg immovable by suitable clamps, strap a board to the bottom of the foot, and connect the toe end of the board with a weight by means of a cord passing over a pulley. As the weight is increased or decreased the foot is more or less flexed, and the gastrocnemius muscle is stretched more or less. A pointer fastened to the foot-board moves over a scale and indicates the amount the muscle changes its length when subjected to various weights. Mosso reports that though the curve of elasticity has about the same character as that of isolated frog muscle, the curve of extensibility is different, each of the added weights causing greater amount of stretching. This is probably due to the fact that a muscle within the body is always being influenced by the central nervous system. Its length at any given moment is due not only to its elasticity as compared with that of its antagonist, but also to the strength of the nervous impulses, reflex and voluntary (often unintentional), coming to it. The sub- ject would have to be under an anaesthetic or in very deep sleep for such an experiment to give a true picture of its elasticity. Mosso describes, in fact, movements of the foot aeeompanying the respirations, due to variations in the tonus impulses coming to the muscles in inspiration and expiration. In spite of the innate difficulties of such an experiment, we can ascertain that in general the conclusions arrived at by studying the isolated muscles of a frog apply to the muscles when in the living body. The elasticity of a muscle within the normal body suffices to preserve the ten- sion of the muscle under all ordinary conditions. The muscles are attached to the bones under elastic tension, as is shown by the separation of the ends in case a muscle be cut. This elastic tension is very favorable to the action of the muscle, as it takes up the slack and ensures that at the instant the muscle begins to shorten the effect of the change shall be quickly imparted to the bones which it is its function to move. The extensibility of the muscle is a great protection, lessening the danger of rupture of the muscle-fibres and ligaments, and the injury of joints when the muscles contract suddenly and vigorously, or when they are subjected to sudden strains by external forces. The importance of extensibility and elasticity to muscles which act as antag- onists is evident. When a muscle suddenly contracts against a resisting force such as the inertia of a heavy weight, the energy of contraction, which puts the muscle on the stretch, is temporarily stored in it as elastic force, and as the weight yields to the strain, is given out again; thus the effect of the contrac- tion force is tempered, the application of the suddenly developed energy being prolonged and softened. Elasticity is very important to the function of the non-striated muscles of the blood-vessels, bladder, intestine, etc. This is especially true of the sphincter muscles, for it is an important factor in securing the continued tension characteristic of their action. 4. Influences which Affect the Activity and Character of the Con- traction.— («) The Character of the Mused-. — Attention has been called to the fact that irritability and conductivity may be different not only in different kinds of muscle-tissue, and in muscles of different animals, but even in similar 108 AX AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. kinds of muscle-tissue iu the different muscles of the same animal; the same may be said of contractility. Although irritability, conductivity, and contrac- tility are to be regarded as different properties of muscle protoplasm, they are usually found to be developed to a corresponding degree in each muscle. Those forms of muscle which require for their excitation irritants of slow and prolonged action, are found to conduct slowly and to make slow and long- drawn-out contractions, and muscles which are excited by irritants acting rapidly and briefly are noted for the quickness with which they contract and relax. Differences in the activity of the contraction process are made evident by the duration of single contractions of different forms of muscle-tissue. The duration of the contraction of the striated muscles of different animals differs greatly, e. g. of the frog -^ second, of the turtle 1 second, of certain insects only -^-^ second. Even muscles of apparently the same kind in the Pectoralia major Omuli yoid //\^\~~~~~'j racUis ^^~~-^^ J • • • • I I Fig. 41.— Records of maximal isotonic contractions of four different muscles from a turtle, each weighted with 30 grams : Pectoralis major ; omohyoid ; gracilis ; palmaris. The dots mark & second, and the longer marks seconds (after Cash).2 same animal exhibit different degrees of activity. Cash1 reports the following differences in the duration of the contractions of different striated muscles of a frog in fractions of a second: Hyoglossus, 0.205; rectus abdominis, 0.170; gastrocnemius, 0.120 j semimembranosus, 0.108 ; triceps femoris, 0.104. Sim- ilar differences are found to exist between different muscles in other animals — in the turtle, for instance, as is shown by the myograms in Fig. 41. It is interesting to connect the rate of the contraction process in different muscles with their function. The omohyoid muscle of the turtle is capable of comparatively rapid contractions, and the action of this muscle is to draw back the head beneath the projecting shell ; the pectoralis, on the other hand, although strong, contracts slowly; it is a muscle of locomotion and has to move the heavy body of the animal. Unstriated muscles, which are remark- able for the slowness and the duration of their contractions, are found chiefly in the walls of the intestines, blood-vessels, etc., which require to remain in a state of continued contraction for considerable periods and do not need to alter rapidly. It is the business of the heart-muscle to drive fluids often against considerable resistance, and a strong, not too rapid, slightly prolonged contrac- tion, such as is peculiar to it, would be best adapted to its function. The bulk of the muscles of the bodies of warm-blooded animals arc capable of rapid contraction and relaxation, but the rate normal to the muscle is found to vary with the form of work to be done. The muscles which control the vocal organs, for instance, have a very rapid rate of relaxation as w^ell as of con- 1 Arrhivfiir Anatomie und Physiologie, 1880, Suppl. Bd., S. 147. ' Op. cit., S. 157. GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. 109 traction. The muscles which move the bones appear to have different rates of contraction and relaxation according to the weight of the parts to be moved ; those which control the lighter parts, as the hand, being capable of rapid con- tractions, while those which have to overcome the inertia of heavier parte, to which rapidity of action would be a positive disadvantage, react more slowly. In general, where rapid, brief, and vigorous contractions are required, pale striated muscles are found; where more prolonged contractions are needed, red striated muscles occur. The accompanying myograms (Fig. 42) illustrate mo Fig. 42. — A, maximal contractions of the gastrocnemius medialis of the rabbit (pale muscle), weighted with 50, 100, 300, and 500 grams ; B, maximal contractions of the soleus of the rabbit (red muscle), weighted with 50, 100, and 200 grams (after Cash). the difference in the rate of contractions of pale and red striated muscles of the rabbit. Ranvier says the latent period of red muscle of rabbit is four times as long as that of the pale ; and Tigerstedt states the latent period of red muscles of the frog to be 0.02 second and of the pale muscles 0.005 second. Pale and red striated fibres are found united in the same muscle in certain instances, and in these eases it is supposed that the former, which are capable of very rapid and powerful but short-lived contractions, start the movement, while the slower red muscles continue it. Bottazzi ' would explain many of the peculiarities of muscle contraction on the theory that both the isotropic and anisotropic substances are contractile, and that they react differently under varving conditions. The isotropic substance, the Barcoplasma, is responsible for the slow, prolonged movements of the muscle and the aniso- tropic substance for the rapid, brief movements. In ordinary contractions they both act, though to different degrees. (6) Effect of Tension Caused by Weights and Myograph-lever on the Extent and Course of the Contraction. — As we have seen, the rate of the contraction of an ordinary striated muscle is much too rapid to be followed by the eye. and to study the course of the change in form it is necessary to employ sonic kind of recording mechanism. Every mechanical device for recording the movements of the muscle has inertia, and, if given motion, acquires momen- 1 Bottazzi : Journalof Physiology, 1897, xxi. p, 1. 110 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. turn. Roth of these factors would tend to alter the shape of the record, and the more the greater the weight of the recording apparatus. A weight, or tension, can be applied to a muscle in various ways, and the form of the contraction will be correspondingly changed. If a muscle is made to work with a considerable weight hanging on it, we speak of it as loaded; if the weight be connected with the muscle, but so supported that it does not pull on it until the muscle begins to shorten, the muscle is said to be after- loaded ; if the weight is the same throughout the contraction, as when the muscle has only to lift a light weight, applied close to the axis of the lever, the contraction is said to be isotonic; if on the other hand the contracting muscle is made to work against a strong spring, so that it can shorten very little, i. e. has almost the same length throughout the contraction, the contraction is said to be isometric.1 The shape of the myogram recorded as a result of the same stimulus would evidently be very different in these four cases. The effect of a weight to alter the myogram is illustrated in the record given in Figure 43. Increasing the weight prolonged the latent period, and lessened the height and duration of the con- tractions. The alterations liable to occur in the form of the myogram by the isotonic method, as a result of the mechanical conditions under which the work is done, are — (1) Prolongation of the latent period. There can be no move- ment of the lever until the inertia of the weight has been overcome, and the first effect of the contrac- tion is to stretch the muscle, a part of the energy of contraction being changed to elastic force, which on the recoil assists in raising the weight. Thus the myogram may fail to reveal the instant that the contraction process starts. Indeed, inasmuch as tension increases the activity of muscle protoplasm, it is probable that the presence of the weight really hastens the liberation of energy at the same time that it delays the recording of the contraction. (2) .1 Iteration in the shape of the ascend in;/ limb of the myograph curve. The weight will either lessen the rate at which the curve rises and decrease the height, or, if the weight be not great, it may acquire a velocity from the energy suddenly imparted to it by the muscle, which will carry the record higher 1 Fick : Mechanische Arbeit und W armeentwickelung bci der Muskelthatigheit, Leipzig, 1882. Fig. 43.— Effect of the weight upon the form of the myogram. The gastrocnemius muscle of a frog excited by maximal breaking induction shocks five times, the weight being increased after each contraction, and in the intervals supported at the normal resting length of the muscle; i. e. the muscle was after-loaded: 1, muscle weighted only with very light lever; 2, weight five grams ; 3, ten grams : 4, twenty-five grams ; 5, fifty grams. The perpendicular line marks the moment of excitation. The time is recorded at the bottom of the curve by a chronograph, actuated by a tuning-fork vibrating 50 times per second. GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. Ill than the absolute contraction of the muscle. The part of the myogram cor- responding to the height of the contraction of the muscle can be distinguished from that due to the throw of the lever by a method suggested by Kaiser.1 If the rising lever strikes a check, it remains in contact with the check as long as the muscle continues to contract, but falls immediately if not held there by the contraction process. By varying the height of the check, the point corresponding to the true contraction height can be ascertained. (3) The fall of the curve may be altered.' The weight, suddenly freed by the rapidly relaxing muscle, may acquire a velocity in falling which will stretch the muscle-tissue, carry the record lower than the actual relaxation of the muscle would warrant, and lead to the development of artificial elastic after- oscillations. It must not be supposed, however, that the relaxation of the muscle is merely a passive affair, and that it returns to its original shape because, when it ceases to develop energy, it is stretched by the weight. The relaxation, like the contraction process, is an active event, and it is antago- nistic to the contraction process.2 These sources of error can be in part overcome by the employment of an exceedingly light, stiff writing-lever, and by bringing the necessary tension on the muscle by placing the extending weight very near the axis of the lever, so that it shall move but little and hence acquire little velocity. (c) Effect of Rate of Excitation on Height and Form of Muscular Contrac- tion.— If a muscle be excited a number of times by exactly the same irritant and under the same external conditions, the amount and course of each of the contractions should be exactly the same, provided the condition of the muscle itself remains the same. The condition of the muscle is, however, altered every time it is excited to contraction, and each contraction leaves behind it an after-effect. This altered condition is not permanent; as we have seen, increased katabolism is accompanied by increased anabolism, and, if the excitations do not follow each other too rapidly, the katabolic changes occur- ring in contraction are compensated for by anabolic changes during the suc- ceeding interval of rest. Normally, a muscle, under the restorative influence of the blood, rapidly recovers from the alterations produced by the contraction process, and, therefore, if not excited too frequently, will give, other things being equal, the same response each time it is called into action. The lust illustration of this is the heart, which continues to beat at a regular rate throughout the life of the individual. Tiege] found that one of the skeletal muscles of a frog, while in the normal body, can make more than a thousand contractions in response to artificial stimuli without showing fatigue; finally the effect of the work shows itself in a lessening of the power to contract. Every muscle contains a surplus of energy-holding compounds and also sub- stances capable of neutralizing waste products, and even a muscle which has been separated from the rest of the body retains tor a considerable time the ability to recover from the effects of excitation. It is evident that when a 1 Kaiser: Zeitechrift far Biologic, 1896, xxxiii. S. 157, 360. 2 Fick, v. K rifs, and others 112 AN AMERICAN TENT-BOOK OE PHYSIOLOGY. muscle is excited repeatedly, a certain interval of rest must be permitted between the succeeding excitation- it* its normal condition is to be maintained, and that the more extensive the chemical changes produced by the excita- tions the longer must be the periods allowed for recovery. This being the case, the rate of excitation and consequent length of the interval of rest will have a great effect upon the condition of the muscle and its eapaeity for work. (1) Eti'cft of Frequent F.reitations on the Height of Separate Muscular ( 'ontracUons. — Other things being equal, the height to which a muscle can con- tract when excited by a given irritant can be taken as an index of its capacity to do work, and if a muscle be excited many times in succession, the effect of action upon the strength of the contraction process, the endurance, and the coming on of fatigue can be estimated from the height of the succeeding con- tractions. One might expect that every contraction would tend to fatigue and to lesseu the power of the muscle, but almost the first effect of action is to increase the irritability and mobility of muscle protoplasm. Introductory and Staircase Contractions. — The peculiar effect of action to increase muscular activity was first observed by Bowditch,1 when studying the effect of excitations upon the heart. He found that repeated excitations of equal strength applied to the ventricle of a frog's heart caused a series of contractions each of which was greater than the preceding. If the contrac- tions were recorded on a regularly moviug surface, the summits of the succes- sive contractions were seeu to rise oue above the other like a flight of steps. This peculiar phenomenon received the name of the " staircase contractions " (see Fig. 1 1). A ■*m 1 1 1 j i Fig. 44 —Staircase contractions of a frog's ventricle in response to a series of like stimuli, written on a regularly revolving drum by the float of a water manometer connected with the chamber of the ventricle (after Bowditch). The record is to be read from right to left. This effect of repeated excitations was later observed by Tiegel,2 on the skeletal muscles of frogs; by Rossbach,3 on the muscles of warm-blooded animals, and by Romanes4 on the contractile tissues of Medusae. The following .-erics of contractions (Fig. 45), which closely resembles the above, was obtained from the gastrocnemius muscle of a frog, excited at a regular rate by a series of equal breaking induction shocks. The contractions in Figure 45 did not begin to increase in height imme- diately ; on the contrary, each of the first four contractions was slightly lower than the one w hich preceded it. A decline in the height of the first three or tour contraction- is the rule when a normal resting muscle is called into action 1 BerichU der koniglichen sach&ischen Gesettsehqft der Wissenschaft, 1871. 9 Pfliiger>8' Archie, 1882, 1884, Bd. xiii.. xv. * Romanes: Jelly-fish und Star-fish, International Science Series, p. 54. 2 Ibid.. 1875. GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. 113 (see Figs. 46 and 49), and these contractions at the beginning of a series have received the name of the " introductory contractions." The introductory con- tractions appear to indicate that the first effect of action is to lessen irritability, or that anabolic changes are too slow to compensate for katabolic changes, and each of the first few contractions leaves behind it a fatigue effect. It is uot long, however, before the influence of activity to heighten anabolism and increase irritability shows itself in the growth of the height of the succeeding contractions, and the " staircase contractions" are observed. This growth of the height of contractions must necessarily reach a limit, and the amount of increase is found to gradually lessen until the succeeding contractions have the same height. Sometimes the full height of the staircase is not reached before more than a hundred contractions have been made. These maximal contractions may be repeated many times ; sooner or later, however, an antagonistic effect of the work manifests itself and the height of the contractions begins to lessen. Effect of Fatigue— A. decline in the height of the contractions is an evidence of fatigue, and indicates that anabolism is failing to keep pace with Fig. 45.— Staircase contractions of gastrocnemius muscle of a frog, excited once every two seconds by strong breaking induction shocks. katabolism, or that the waste products which result from the work are col- lecting faster than they can be removed or neutralized and are exerting a paralyzing influence on the muscle protoplasm (see p. 70). From this time on, the height of the succeeding contractions continually lessens, and often with great regularity, so that a line drawn so as to connect the summits of the declining contractions, the "curve of fatigue," as it is called, may be a straight line. In the experiment, parts of the record of which are reproduced in Figure 46, an isolated gastrocnemius muscle of a frog was excited with maximal breaking induction shocks at the rate of 25 times a minute for about one and one-half hours; the contractions were isotonic, and the total weight of lever and load did not exceed 20 grains ; the records of the succeeding contractions were recorded on a slowly moving cylinder. The experiment consisted of two parts — in the first (i(J contractions, in the second over 1700 contractions were made; an interval of rest of five minutes was permitted between the two series. In the first part of the experiment there was a decline in the height of the contractions tor tin; first five contractions, the "introductory contractions," then during the next sixty-one contractions a gradual rise in the height of the Vol. II.— 8 114 AN AMERICAN TENT-BOOK OE PHYSIOLOGY. contractions, the " staircase contractions." These phenomena repeat themselves in the second part of the experiment, that following the interval of rest. The contractions at the beginning- of the second series were not so high as those at the end of the first series, though somewhat higher than those seen at the beginning of the first series; the rest of five minutes was not sufficient to entirely do away with the stimulating influence of the preceding work. The contractions of the second series took the following course: The first four introductory contractions gradually declined, then came the staircase contrac- tions, which continued to rise until the 100th contraction, when a gradual lessening of the height of the contractions began. This decline continued 6fi contractions. Rest. 1-30 400 500 G00 700 S00 900 1000 1100 1200 1300 1100 1500 1(300 1700 Fig. 46.— Effect of fatigue on the height of muscular contractions. The figure is a reproduction of parts of a record of over 1700 contractions made hy an isolated gastrocnemius muscle of a frog. The con- tractions were isotonic, the weight heing about 20 grams. The stimuli were maximal breaking induction shocks, and were applied directly to the muscle, at the rate of 25 per minute. Between the first group of 66 contractions and the following groups a rest of five minutes was given; after this rest the work was continued without interruption for about one and a half hours. The second group of contractions, that immediately following the period of rest, contains the tirst twenty contractions of the new series; the next group the 100th to the 110th ; the next the 200th to the 210th, and so on. throughout the long series of more than 1700 contractions given in the record, and, had the experiment been continued, would have undoubtedly gone on until the power was completely lost. The curve of fatigue was not a straight line, but fell somewhat more rapidly during the early part of the work than toward the end. That the peculiar changes in the height of the contractions which occur in the early part of an experiment such as that which we have described are not abnormal, and the result of the artificial conditions under which the work is done, is shown not only by the fact that they are observed when a muscle (1ENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. 115 which has its normal blood-supply is rhythmically excited to a large Dumber of contractions, but by the personal experience of every one accustomed to violent muscular exercise. Everyone is conscious that he cannot put out the greatest muscular effort until he has " warmed up to the work." The runner precedes the race by a short run; the oarsman takes a short pull before going to the line; in all the sports one sees the contestants making movements to •'limber up" before they enter upon the work of the game. These prelim- inary movements are performed not only to put the muscles in better c lition for action, but to ensure more accurate co-ordination — that is to say, the facts ascertained for the muscle can be carried over to the central nervous system. The finely adjusted activities of the nerve-cells which control the muscles reach their perfection only after repeated action. In such experiments as that recorded in Figure 46 the record shows to Fir,. 47.— Effect of excitation upon the form of separate contractions. In this experiment the records of the muscular contractions were taken upon a rapidly revolving drum. The muscle was the gas- trocnemius of the frog; the contractions were Isotonic ; the weight was very light, about 10 grams; the stimuli were maximal breaking induction shocks; and the rate of stimulation was twenty-three per minute. 1 marks the first contraction; 2, the 100th; ;:, the 200th ; I, the 300th. The muscle was excited automatically by an arrangement carried by the drum, and the excitation whs always given when a definite part of the surface of the drum was opposite the point of the lever which recorded the con- tractions. a remarkable degree the fact that at any given time the muscle has a definite capacity for work. A suitable explanation of this is lacking. The corre- spondence in the height of the contractions of the same group, and the differ- ence in the height of different groups of contractions, must be attributed to the existence within the muscle-cell of some automatic mechanism which regulates the liberation of energy and which has its activity greatly influenced by the alterations which result from action. Whether this supposed automatic regu- latory mechanism controls both the preparation of the final material from which the energy displayed by the muscle is liberated, and the amount of the explosive change which results from the application of the irritant, cannot be definitely said. (2) Effect of Frequent Excitations upon the Form of Separate Contractions. — The effect of activity is not only observable in the change in the height 116 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. of the muscular contractions, but in the length of the Intent period, in the rate at which the muscle shortens, and in the rate at which the muscle relaxes. The etfect of a large number of separate contractions, made in quick succes- sion, upon the rate at which the muscle changes its form during contraction, is illustrated in the myograms reproduced in Figure 47. In Figure 47 only the 1st, 100th, 200th, and 300th contractions were re- corded. The perpendicular line marks the point at which the stimulus was given. In this experiment the latent period for each of the succeeding con- tractions is seen to be longer ; the height is lessened ; the rise of the curve of contraction is slowed and the curve of relaxation is even more prolonged. These and certain other changes are to be observed in the records of Figure 48, which were taken in an experiment made under the same conditions as the last, except that the rate of excitation was 80 per minute, instead of 23, as in the preced- ing experiment, and the record of every 50th contraction was recorded. Fig. 48.— Effect of frequent excitation on the form of separate contractions. The method employed to obtain this record is the same as in the preceding experiment, except that the drum is revolving more rapidly, and every 50th contraction is recorded : 1 marks the first contraction ; 2, the 50th : 3, the 100th ; 4, the 150th ; 5, the 200th ; 6, the 250th ; 7, the 300th. A comparison of the first with the 50th contraction gives a number of points of interest. The stimulating effect of action upon the contraction pro- cess is shown by the fact that the latent period of the 50th (2 of Fig. 48) is shorter than that of the first, the rise of the curve is somewhat steeper, and the height is considerably greater. It is noticeable, however, that the crest is prolonged, and consequently the total length of the contraction is increased. Such a prolongation of the contraction is known as "Contracture." In con- sidering the greater activity of the contraction process of this 50th contraction as compared with the first, we must recall that it represents one of a series of staircase contractions, such as we noticed in Figure 46. If we examine the 100th contraction (3 of Fig. 48), we see the evidences of the beginning of fatigue ; although the latent period is nearly as quick as in the first, the rise of the curve is less rapid, the height is less, and rate of relaxation is very much slowed. These changes are to be seen in a more marked degree in the L50th contraction (4 of Fig. 48), and the prolongation of the crest of the* contraction and the decreased rate of relaxation are particularly noticeable. The same sort of differences is to be observed in the later contractions. By GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. 117 still more rapid rates of excitation these alterations in the contraction curve are not only exaggerated, but develop more quickly, and play a very important part in producing the peculiar form of continued contraction known as tetanus. Lee l states that the slowing of the contraction process, which is compara- tively slight in the muscles of the frog, is very marked in the muscles of the turtle, but practically absent from the white muscles of the cat. Moreover, the prolongation of the relaxation which is very noticeable in the case of the muscles of frogs and turtles, is very slight in those of the cat. Contracture effects have, however, been seen on both the red and pale muscles of the rabbit and on the muscles of man. Although the muscles of different animals show certain peculiarities, the facts illustrated in the above experiments can be considered as in general true of most striated muscles. (3) Effect of Frequent Excitations to Produce Tetanus. — As we have seen, the normal muscle the first time that it is excited relaxes almost as quickly as it contracts, but if it be excited rhythmically a number of times a minute, gradu- ally loses its power of rapid relaxation. The tendency to remain contracted begins to show itself in a prolongation of the crest of the contraction curve, even before fatigue comes on, and increases for a considerable time in spite of the effect of fatigue in lessening the height of the contractions. If a skeletal mus- cle of a frog be excited many times, at a rate of about once every two seconds, the gradual increase in the duration of the contractions will have the effect of preventing the muscle from returning to its normal length in the intervals be- tween the succeeding stimuli, for contraction will be excited before relaxation is complete. As is shown in the record of the experiment reproduced in Figure 49, there will come a time in the work when the base-line connecting the lower extremities of the succeeding myograms will be seen to rise in the form of a curve, the change being at first gradual, then more and more rapid, and then again gradual (see b, Fig. 49). The effect of the change in the power to relax is to make it appear as if the muscle were the seat of two contraction processes, the one acting continuously, the other intermittently in response to the suc- cessive excitations. Such a condition as that exhibited in section c, Figure 1!'. is spoken of as an incomplete tetanus, complete tetanus being a condition of continuous contraction caused by rhythmical excitations, in which none of the separate contraction movements are visible. In complete tetanus the muscle writes an unbroken curve. The slowing of the relaxation of the muscle and consequent state of con- tinued shortening which is to be seen in the latter part of the above experiment is the result of the developing contracture. The amount of contracture increases, within limits, with the increase in the strength and rate of exci- tation. The intensity and rate of stimulation required for the production of this condition depend very largely upon the character of the muscle and its condition at tin' time. In the experiment recorded in Figure 50 the development of the condition of contracture was more marked than in the 1 Lee : American Journal of I'hytiology, 1899, ii. 3, p. 11. 118 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. above experiment, and the resulting condition of continued contraction caused first incomplete and finally complete tetanus. Although frequent excitations appear to be essential to the development of contracture, it is not to be considered a fatigue effect, since the contracted state which it produces may be increasing at the time that fatigue is lessen- in"- the height of the ordinary contraction movements, and since the form of contraction peculiar to contracture is itself seen to lessen as fatigue becomes excessive. Both of these facts are illustrated in Figure 50, but are more strikingly shown in Figure 51, in which a more rapid rate of excitation was used. The effect of fatigue to prolong muscular contractions and the relation of contracture to fatigue effects will be considered later (sec p. 130). The record in Figure 51 shows many points of interest : a to b, a rapidly Fig. 49.— Effect of frequent stimuli to gradually produce incomplete tetanus. Series of isotonic con- tractions of a gastrocnemius muscle of a frog, excited once every two seconds by strong breaking induc- tion shocks. Only a part of the record is shown, 70 contractions have been omitted between the end of the section marked o and the beginning of section b, and 200 contractions between the end of section band tin beginning of c. The increase in the extent of the relaxations seen at the close of the record was due to the slowing of the rate of excitations at that time. developing staircase, which is accompanied by a rising of the base line, which indicates that contracture began to make itself felt from the moment the work began ; b to c, a rapid and then a gradual fall in the height of contractions due to fatigue effects ; c to d, a rise in the top of the curve in spite of the lessening height of the contractions, due to the increasing contracture ; d to e, GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. 1 1 5> a gradual fall of the curve of incomplete tetanus, due to the effect of fatigue on the contracture; e, complete tetanus, but continued gradual declme in the height of the curve under the influence of fatigue. Fig. 50.— Effect of frequent excitations to gradually produce tetanus. Experiment on a gastrocnemius muscle of a frog, similar to the last. The weight was only 10 grams. The rate of excitation was 100 per minute. This muscle had been worked a short time before this series of contractions was taken, and, as a result, the introductory and staircase contractions were absent and contracture began much sooner than in the experiment recorded in Figure 48. The record in section b is a continuation of that in section a. The following experiment, Figure 52, differs from those which have preceded it, in that the muscle, instead of being directly excited, was stimulated indirect ly by irritation of its nerve. Each shock applied to the nerve was represented by a separate contraction process in the muscle. The experiment illustrates well the combined effect of the staircase and the contractu re to raise the height Fig. 51.— Developmenl anil fatigue of contracture, Experiment <>n a gastrocnemius muscle of a frog. The weight was 10 grams. U In the preceding experiments strong maximal breaking induction shocks were used to excite. The rate of excitation was 5 per second, The record appears as a silhouette for the reason that the drum was moving very slowly. of the contractions. On account of the more rapid rate of excitation, the contracture came on more quickly than in the preceding experiments ; it did 120 AX AMERICAN TEXT- BO OK OF PHYSIOLOGY. not become sufficient during the few seconds that this experiment lasted to prevent the separate relaxations from being seen, and an incomplete tetanus was the result. In the experiment the record of which is given in Figure 53, the muscle was directly stimulated, and the rate of excitation was rapid, 33 per second. Not even this rate sufficed to cause complete tetanus, and the crest of the curve Fig. 52.— Development of incomplete tetanus and contracture, by indirect stimulation. A gas- trocnemius muscle of a frog was indirectly stimulated by breaking induction shocks, of medium strength, applied to the sciatic nerve. The rate was about 8 per second, as shown by comparison of the seconds traced at the bottom of the figure with the oscillations caused by the separate contractions. The weight was somewhat heavier than in the preceding experiment. The drum was revolving much faster than in the other experiments, hence the difference in the apparent duration of the contractions. shows fine waves, which represent the separate contractions the combined effect of which resulted in the almost unbroken curve seen in the record. Had the rate been a little more rapid, no waves could have been detected and the tetanus would have been complete from the start. The effects of the staircase and con- tracture are merged into one another, and a very rapid high rise of the curve of contraction is the result. It is noticeable that the summit of the curve is rising throughout the experiment, owing to the increasing contracture. It is- evident that the condition of contracture which is developed in a rapidly stimulated muscle will tend to maintain a condition of continuous con- Fig. 53.— Effect of rapid excitations to produce tetanus. Experiment with a gastrocnemius muscle of a frog, excited directly, with breaking induction shocks of medium strength, at the rate of 33 per second. The weight was about 15 grams. The drum was moving much more slowly than in the pre- ceding experiment. The time record gives fiftieths of a second. traction, there being no opportunity for the muscle to relax in the intervals between the succeeding excitations. 4. Explanation of the Great Height of Tetanic Contractions. — We have now to seek an explanation of the fact that a muscle when tetanized will con- GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. 121 tract much higher than it will as a result of a single excitation. As we have seen, repeated excitations cause, in the case <»('a fresh muscle, a gradual increase in irritability and consequently a gradual rise in the height of the succeeding contractions, but the staircase sooner or later reaches its upper limit, and will not alone account for the great shortening which occurs in tetanus. Effect of Two Rapidly Following Excitations. — Helmholtz was the first to investigate the effect of rate of excitation on the height of combined contrac- tions. For the sake of simplicity, he excited a muscle with only two breaking induction shocks, of the same strength, and observed the effect of varying the interval between these two excitations. He concluded that if the second stim- ulus is given during the latent period of the first contraction, the effect is the same as if the muscle has received but one shock ; if the second shock be applied at some time during the contraction excited by the first, the second contraction behaves as if the amount of contraction present when it begins were the resting state of the muscle, i. e. the condition of activity caused by the first shock has no influence on the amount of activity caused by the second, but the h< aght of the second contraction is simply added to the amount of the first contraction present. Were this rule correct, as a result of this summation, if the second contraction occurred when the first was at its height, the sum of the two con- tractions would be double the height of either contraction taken by itself. Helmholtz' conclusion, that the condition of activity awakened by the first excitation has no effect upon that caused by the second excitation, has not been substantiated by later observers. Von Kries ' has found that the presence of the first contraction hastens the development of the contraction process result- ing from the second excitation ; and Von Frey 2 has ascertained that Helm- holtz's rule of summation applies only to weighted muscles. In the case of unweighted muscles the summation effect is greatest when the second contrac- tion starts during the period of developing energy caused by the first excita- tion, i. e. during the rise of the first contraction. If the second contraction Fig. 54.— A schema i 'i' Hi. effect Of double excitations upon the gracilis muscle of n froc, by di Hi-r- ent intervals of excitation. To obtain ibis figure, the results of different experiments were super- imposed (after Von Frey). starts during the period of relaxation of the first, the second may be not even as high as when occurring alone (see Fig. 54). 1 Archiv Jiir Anatomic und J'hysiologie, 1888. 2 Ibid., S. 213. 122 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OE PHYSIOLOGY The fact that the second contraction is higher if it starts during the ascent of the first, may be explained as due to a summation of the condition of ex- Fk;. 55.— Effect of support on height of contractions (after Von Frey) : a, gastrocnemius muscle of a frog, separate contractions, tetanus, separate contractions, and group of supported contractions ; weight 10.5 grams ; 6, the same, by weight of 0.5 grams. citation awakened by the two irritants, and hence the liberation of a greater amount of energy. Nevertheless, the increased irritability, indicated by stair- case contractions, and the summation of excitation effects which occur by rapidly repeated excitations, shown by the above experiment, do not suffice to wholly explain the great shortening of the muscle seen in tetanus. Helmholtz' idea, that there is a support afforded by the first contraction to the second, must also play an important part, and we must turn to this for the completion of the explanation of the great height acquired by the tetanus curve. Effect of Support on the Height of Contractions. — Von Kries1 and Von Frey2 found that, in general, the shorter the distance the muscle has. to raise a weight, the higher it can contract, and that if a muscle be excited at a regu- lar rate, and the support for the weight be raised between each of the succeed- ing contractions, at a certain height of the support the contractions may be as high as during tetanus (see Fig. 55) This effect can be got with a fresh muscle when the interval between the excitations is such that there can be no summation in Helmholtz' sense. The importance of this discovery to our understanding of tetanus is very great, for it has been found that if an unsupported muscle be rapidly excited, effects are observed which closely resemble those obtained by the aid of a sup- port; this we have seen in the experiments recorded in Figures 50, 51, p. 115*. After a certain amount of excitation, a change occurs in the condition of a muscle, owing to which it acts as if it had received an upward push, and as if a new force had been developed within it, which aids the ordinary con- traction process in raising the weight. The new aid to high contraction is the support afforded by the developing condition of contracture. That con- 1 Archir Jar Analmnie. und 1'hy.vnlor/ie, 188(5. - Ibid., 1887. GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. 123 tracture offers an internal support to the muscle, and raises the total height of the contraction curve just as von Frey found an external support to do, can be seen in Figure 57. 5. Effect of Gradually Increasing the Rate of Excitation. — One of the most instructive methods of exciting tetanus is to send into the muscle a series of breaking induction shocks of medium intensity, at a gradually increasing rate. The record of such an experiment has been reproduced in Figure 56. Fig. 56.— Effect of a gradually increasing rate of excitation. Excitation of a gastrocnemius muscle of a frog with breaking induction shocks of medium strength, The time was recorded directly, by a tuning-fork making 100 vibrations per second. The rate of excitation was gradually increased, and then gradually decreased. The ascending curve, n-b, shows the effect of increasimr, and the descending curve, c-d, of decreasing the rate of stimulation. Excitation was given by means of a special mechanism for interrupting the primary circuil of an induction apparatus and at t he same time short-circuiting the making shocks. This interrupter was run by an electric motor Which was allowed to Speed lip slowly, and was slowed down gradually. At the beginning of the experiment, a, one complete contraction with a wave of elastic after-vibration was recorded j this was followed by two eon- tractions of le.-s height, " introductory contractions ;" then came three contrac- tions each of which was higher than the preceding, "staircase contractions;" these were followed by three contraction.-, which, in spite .,1" the developing contracture, were of less height, " fatigue effect" The rate of excitation at this place was about 17 per second. From this point on, the developing con- tracture supported the muscle more and more and the contraction waves became less and less, until finally, when the rate had become ,">f Excitations required to Tetanize. — The number of stimuli per second required to tetanize a muscle depends largely on the nature of the 126 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OE PHYSIOLOGY. muscle, for this divides the character of the separate contractions, and, through them, the effect of their combined action. The duration of the separate contractions, and the tendency of the muscle to enter into contracture, are the predominant factors in determining the result. ( !omplete tetanus can only be obtained in the case of a fresh muscle, when the interval between succeeding stimuli is shorter 'than is required for the muscle to reach its maximal contraction by a single stimulus. Thus the prolonged contractions of smooth muscles permit of the development of a form of tetanus by successive closures of the galvanic current at intervals of several seconds. The non-striated muscle of the bladder of the cat can be tetanized by induc- tion shocks given at a rate of a little less than one in two seconds.1 The contraction of some of the muscles of the turtle may last nearly a second, and two or three excitations a second suffice to tetanize. The muscles of mar-* mots during the winter sleep can be tetanized by 5 excitations per second (Patrizi). Tetanus of the red (slowly contracting) striated muscles of the rabbit can be obtained by 10 excitations per second, while 20-30 per second are required to tetanize the pale (active) striated muscles (Kronecker and Sterling) ; 100 stimuli per second are needed to tetanize the muscles of some birds (Richet), and over 300 per second would be required to tetanize the muscles of some insects (Marey). Any influence that will prolong the contrac- tion process will lessen the rate of excitation required to tetanize. 8. Effect of Exceedingly Rapid Excitations. — The question arises, Is there an upper limit to the rate of excitation to which muscles will respond by tetanus? There is no doubt that this is the case, but there is a difference of opinion as to what the limit is, and how it shall be explained. Striated muscles and nerves can be excited by rates at which our most deli- cate chronographs fail to act. The muscle ceases to be tetanized by direct exci- tation at a rate by which it can still be indirectly excited through its nerve. The highest rate for the nerve has been placed at from 3000 to 22,000 by differ- ent observers,2 and this wide difference is probably attributable to the methods of excitation employed. That such different results should have been reached is not strange, if we recall the many conditions upon which the exciting power of the irritant depends. That tetanus should be obtained by such high rates docs not show that the nerve responds to each of the separate shocks. As a rule, when the rate of excitation is so high that tetanus fails a contraction is observed when the current is thrown into the nerve, and often another when it is withdrawn from the nerve — that is, the muscle behaves as if it were sub- jected to a continuous battery current. A satisfactory explanation for this, as well as for the failure of the tetanus, is at present lacking. 9. Relative Intensity <>/ Tetanus and Single ( 'ontntctions. — The amount that a muscle is capacle of shortening, when tetanized by maximal excitations, and 1 C. C. Stewart: American Journal of Physiology, 191)0, iii. p. 25. 1 Kronecker and Sterling: Archivfiir Anatomie und Physiologic, 1878, and Journal of Physi- ology, 1880, vol. i. Von Frey nnd Wiedermann: Bcrichtc der sachsischen Gesellschaft der Wissen- schuft, 1885. Roth: Pjluga's A rchiv, 1888. GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. 127 the strength of the tetanic contraction, depends very largely on the kind of muscle. For example, pale striated muscles, although capable of higher and more rapid single contractions than the red striated, do nol show as great an increase in the height and strength of contractions when tetanized as do th< red; the latter, which are very rich in sarcoplasma, have likewise the greatei endurance. Gruetzner has called them " tetanus muscles," since they seem to be particularly adapted to this form of contraction. Fick found that human muscles when tetanized develop ten times the amount of ten-ion, by isometric contractions, that they give by single contractions ; and in this respect they can be said to resemble red striated muscles. The following relations have been found to exist between the strength of separate contractions and tetanus in certain muscles : triceps and gastrocnemius of the frog, 1 : 2 or 3 ; the cor- responding muscles of the turtle, 1:5; hyoglossus and rectus abdominalis of the frog, 1 : 8 or 9.1 It is evident that no just estimate of the part played by different groups of muscles in the movement of the body can be reached without a careful analysis of the nature of the contractions peculiar to each of the muscles participating in the movement. Both the height and strength of the tetanus is controlled by the intensity of the stimulus. A strong stimulus not only causes the separate contractions of which the tetanus is composed to be higher, but is favorable to the develop- ment of all the other factors which have been described as entering into the pro- duction of tetanus. All normal physiological contractions are supposed to be tetani, and everyone is conscious of the wonderful accuracy with which he can grade the extent and strength of his voluntary movements. The remarkable shading of the intensity of action observable in co-ordinated movements nin-t find its explanation in the adjustment of protoplasmic activity in the nerve- cells of the central nervous system. 10. Continuous Contractions as if the relaxation process had received a sudden check; or, after relaxing quite rapidly for a short time, it may, without having received any visible stimulus, contract again for a short distance and remain so contracted for a considerable time. In any case when the relaxation period is unusually long, the condition of prolonged contraction is termed "contracture." The form of contracture which we are considering at present originates in the muscle itself, and is to be sharply distinguished from a form of pathological contract- ure, which originates in the central nervous system and in which the muscle is kept continuously contracted by impulses coming from the spinal cord. There are a great variety of conditions under which muscles respond to 1 Biedermann : ElektrophysMogie, S. 109. 128 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. excitation by prolonged contractions. If a muscle be excited by frequent induction shocks, oven at a rate insufficient to produce tetanus, after a time it will take on a condition of continuous contraction, which may be main- tained for sonic time after the excitations have ceased (see Fig. 50). If the muscle be very irritable, the contraction caused by a single irritation may be long drawn out. A muscle poisoned by veratria — and the same is true of some other drugs (see p. 137) — may show a remarkable degree of contracture as a result of a single excitation. The contractions of fatigued muscles tend to be greatly prolonged ; and this is very markedly the case with a dying muscle, which gives well-defined, long-continued contractions, localized at the point excited, called by Schiff the " idio-muscular contraction." The contractions caused by the making and breaking of a strong battery current applied to a muscle may likewise be followed by localized contractions which last a considerable time. In this connection one must bear in mind that the length of muscles varies with their elasticity (see p. 105), and that this changes not a little under varying conditions. Finally, it is necessary to recall that muscles when entering into rigor mortis or rigor caloris take on a condition of contraction which may last for days (see p. 159). Contracture in Normal Muscles following Frequent Excitations. — The con- dition of prolonged after-contraction which results from frequent excitations was first studied with care on the muscles of the frog, by Tiegel,1 who gave it the name of " contracture." Richet found that the claw-muscles of the crab are particularly subject to this form of contraction, Rossbach observed it in the muscles of the cat, and Mosso 2 saw it in the muscles of man when vigorously excited either volun- tarily or electrically. Mosso finds a teleological reason for its existence in that it appears most marked under conditions when prolonged contractions are desirable, and might offer a certain economy in the innervation of muscle l>\ lessening the work of the nerve-cell. Richet3 writes that normal con- tracture is not to be confused with the prolonged relaxation of fatigued and dving muscles, nor with the contraction of muscle substance in rigor mortis ; it is best seen on muscles which are fresh and excitable. Although most readily called out by strong direct electrical excitation of the muscle, it is not due to the effect of the current as such, because it may be produced by exciting the muscle indirectly through its nerve, and by voluntary muscular contractions of man. On the other hand, the presence of the nerve is not essential, for curarized muscles may exhibit contracture. That a condition of increased excitability is favorable to the development of contracture is made evident by the curve reproduced in Figure 57. In this experiment the muscle was subjected to a tetanizing induction current for nine seconds, the stimulation being interrupted for an instant every two 1 Tiegel : PfHiget^a Archiv, 1876, xiii. S. 71-84. J Mosso: Archives itcdiennea de Biolorjie, 1890, xiii. pp. 165-179. s Richet: Dictionnairc de Physinlogie, 1899, t. iv. pp. 391-393. GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. 129 seconds, to permit the contracture which was present at these times to show itself. The effect was to increase the excitability of the muscle, as shown by the increased height of the contractions recorded after the tetanus, and to produce a marked contracture, as was shown by the fact that the muscle only partially relaxed after the tetanizing current had ceased, and kept partially contracted in the intervals betweeu the succeeding separate contractions. The fact that contracture can develop hand in hand with increasing excitability shows that it may occur in the absence of fatigue. It is interesting to note that the muscle made contraction and relaxation movements at the same time that it remained continually, although incompletely, contracted ; and finally, that the contracture offered a firm, elastic support to the separate contraction movements, and that the relaxation movements following these separate contractions were rapid, as is made evident by the character of the elastic oscillations resulting from the rapid fall of the lever. The fact that a muscle can remain continuously, though incompletely, con- tracted, at the same time that it makes rapid contraction and relaxation move- ments, suggests that it may at the same time be the seat of two independent contraction processes. The observation recalls the action of the heart mus- cle, for the ventricle maintains a condition of greater or less tonus, at the same time that it makes separate beats ; it is therefore in harmony with a well-known physiological process. Contracture following Single Excitations. — An examination of the contract- ure effects sometimes seen to follow single excitations of irritable muscles throws some light on the nature of the process. Richet observed on the closing-muscle of the claw of the crab that a single excitation caused a rapid contraction, which was followed by a rapid relaxation, and this in turn by a second contraction movement which lasted a considerable time. A similar curve maybe obtained from the striated muscle of a frog incom- pletely poisoned with veratria ; if a single shock be given, the curve rises suddenly, and this quick rise is followed by an immediate fall, which is inter- rupted by a second and slower rise, which is continued as a prolonged con- traction. In both cases the curve suggests thai the single excitation called out two contraction movements, the first a rapid, short-lived contraction, the second a slower, prolonged contraction. It has been suggested that the mus- cle contains two kinds of muscle-fibres, which, like the pale (rapidly con- tracting radialis externus) and red (slowly contracting radialis interims) muscles of the rabbit, have two different rates of contraction.1 This expla- nation is not very satisfactory, because it has been (bund that both the pale and the red muscles of the rabbit can give typical veratria contracture curves.1 Moreover, both heart-muscle and non-striated muscles show independent tonus and contraction movements though containing only one kind of muscle-fibre.3 1 Grutzncr: Pfluget>6 Archiv, 1887, Bd. 41, S. 256. 2 ( 'iirvallo and Weiss: Journal we can artificially excite normal muscles to continuous contraction only by means of a series of rapidly following stimuli, we find it hard to explain continuous physiological contractions on any other basis, and hence the view that the excitation sent by the nerve-cells to muscles has always a rhythmic character, and that the normal motor-nerve impulse is a discontinuous rather than continuous form of excitation. The view is prob- ably correct, but cannot be considered as proved. The evidence in favor of it is as follows : Muscle-sounds, Tremors, etc. — During voluntary muscular contractions the muscle gives out a sound, which would imply that its finest particles are not in a state of equilibrium, but vibrating. By delicate mechanisms it has been possible to obtain records of voluntary and reflex contractions which showed oscillations, although the contraction of the muscle appeared to the eye to be 1 Dictionnaire de Physiologie, 1899, iv. p. 393. GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. 133 continuous. If the surface of a muscle be exposed and be wet and glistening, the light reflected from it during continued contractions is seen to flicker, as if the surface were shaken by fine oscillations. In fatigue the muscle passes from apparently continuous contraction to one exhibiting tremors, and mus- cular tremors are observed under a variety of pathological conditions. With these facts in mind, a number of observers have endeavored to dis- cover the rate at which the muscle is normally stimulated. Experiments in which muscles have been excited to incomplete tetanic contractions by induced currents, interrupted at different rates, have shown that the muscle follows the rate of excitation with a corresponding number of vibrations, and does not show a rate of vibration peculiar to itself. Further, it has been ascertained that the sound given out by a muscle excited to complete tetanus, i. e. an apparently continuous contraction, corresponds to the rate at which it is ex- cited. Apparently, any rate of oscillations detected in a muscle during normal physiological excitation would be an indication of the rate of discharge of impulses from the central nerve-cells. Wollaston was the first to observe that a muscle gives a low dull sound when it is voluntarily contracted, and that this sound corresponds to a rate of vibration of 36 to 40 per second. It may be heard with a stethoscope placed over the contracting biceps muscle, for instance, or if, when all is still and the ears are stopped, one vigorously contracts his masseter muscles. Helmholtz placed vibrating reeds consisting of little strips of paper, etc., on the muscle, and found that only those which had a rate of vibration of 18 to 20 per second were thrown into oscillation when the muscle was voluntarily contracted. This observation indicated that the muscle had a rate of vibration of 18 to 20 per second, a rate too slow to be recognized as a tone. He concluded that the tone heard from the voluntarily contracted muscle was the overtone, instead of the true muscle-tone. The consideration that the resonance tone of the ear itself corresponds to 36 to 40 vibrations per second, makes it question- able whether the muscle-sound should be accepted as evidence of the rate of normal physiological excitation ; nervetheless, the experiments with the vibrating reeds remain to indicate 18 to 20 per second to be the normal rate. Within the last few years a number of researches bearing upon this question have been published, and the results of these point to a still slower rate of vol- untary excitation, varying from 8 to 12 per second according to the muscle on which the experiment is made. Loven1 discovered in the tetanus excited in frogs poisoned with strychnia, and in voluntary contractions, both by mechani- cal methods and by recording the electrical changes occurring during action with the capillary electrometer, rates of 7 to 9 per second. Horsley and Schafer2 excited the brain cortex and motor tracts in the corona radiata and the spinal cord of mammals by induct ion shocks, at widely differing rates, and recorded the resulting muscular contractions by tambours placed over the muscles. They observed oscillations in the myograms obtained which had a 1 Cenlralblatt for die medicinischen fVissenschaften, 1881. 2 Journal of Physiology, 1886, vii. p. 96. 134 AN AMERICAN TEXT- HOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. rate of 8 to 12 per second, the average being 10. The rate of oscillations was. quite independent of the rate of excitation, and oscillations of the same rate were seen by voluntary and by reflex contractions. Tunstall1 found by the use of tambours, in experiments on voluntary contract ions of men, a rate of 8 to 13 per second, with an average of 10. Griffiths2 likewise used the tambour method, and studied the effect of tension on the rate of oscillations in voluntarily contracted human muscles. He observed rates varying from 8 to 19, the rate being increased with an increase of weight up to a certain point, and beyond this decreased. The oscillations became more extensive as fatigue developed. Von Kries by a similar method found rates varying witli different muscles, but averaging about 10. It is not easy to harmonize the view that 8 to 13 excitations per second can cause voluntary tetani, when it is possible for the expert pianist to make as many as 10 or 11 separate movements of the finger in a second. It is, indeed, a common observation that a muscle can be slightly and continuously voluntarily contracted, and, at the same time, be capable of making additional short rapid movements. Von Kries would explain this as due to a peculiar method of innervation, while Biedermann favors Gruetzner's3 view that the muscle may contain two forms of muscle-substance, one of which is slow to react, resembling red muscle-tissue, and maintains the continuous contraction, the other, of more rapid action, being responsible for the quicker movements. Although the evidence is, on the whole, in favor of the view that all normal contractions of voluntary muscles are tetanic in character, there is a great deal which remains to be explained. Effect of Artificial compared with Normal Stimulation. — Experiment shows that, with the same strength of irritant, a muscle contracts more vigorously when irritated indirectly, through its nerve, than when it is directly stimulated. Rosenthal describes the following experiment : If the nerve of muscle A be allowed to rest on a curarized muscle B, and an electric shock be applied in such a way as to excite nerve A and muscle B to the same amount, muscle A will be found to contract more than muscle B. Further, it has been found that muscles respond more vigorously to volun- tary excitations than to any artificial stimulus which can be applied to either the nerve or muscle. This shows itself, not only in the fact that a muscle can by voluntary stimulation lift much larger weights than by electrical excitation, but that after a human muscle has been fatigued by electrical excitations it can still respond vigorously to the will. An illustration of this is given in Figure f>8. Fatigue of Voluntary Muscular Contractions. — Mosso and his pupils have done a large amount of work' upon the fatigue of human muscles when excited by voluntary and artificial stimuli under varying conditions (see p. 72). The results at which they arrived all favor the view that human muscles differ but little from those of warm-blooded animals, and that the facts which have 1 Journal of Physiology, 1886, vii. p. 114. 2 Journal of Physiology, 1888, ix p. 39. 5 Pjluger\ Archiv, 1887, Bd. 41, S. 277. GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. 135 been ascertained by experiments upon cold-blooded animals, such as the frog, can be accepted with but slight modifications for the muscles of man. In the experiment recorded in Figure 58 we see the effect of repeated tetanic Fig. 58.— Voluntary excitations are more effective than electrical. The flexor muscles of the second finger of the left hand of a man were excited first voluntarily, a, then electrically, a-b, and then volun- tarily, b. The electrical excitation consisted of series of induction shocks, which were applied once every two seconds, during about half a second, the spring interrupter of the induction coil vibrating 23 times per second. Each time the muscle contracted it raised a weight of one kilogram. Each of the contractions recorded, whether the result of electrical or voluntary excitation, was a short tetanus. contractions, excited by electricity, to fatigue a human muscle. Normal voluntary contractions, if frequently repeated, provided the muscle has to raise a considerable weight, likewise cause fatigue. This was illustrated in the experiment recorded in Figure 59. Fig. 59.— Effect of fatigue on voluntary muscular contractions. The flexor muscles of the second finger of left hand were voluntarily contracted once every two seconds, and always with the utmost force. The weight raised was four kilograms. It is doubtful whether, in an experiment such as is shown in Figure 59, the loss of the power to raise the weight is due to fatigue of the muscles. It is more likely that the decline in power is due to fatigue of the central nerve- 136 AN AMERICAN TEX1-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. cells by which the muscles are excited to action during the voluntary mus- cular work.1 This fact, that the nerve-cells give out before the muscles, ex- plains the apparent contradiction, that a muscle fatigued by electric excitations can he voluntarily contracted, and when the power to voluntarily contract the muscles has been stopped by fatiguing voluntary work the muscles will respond to electrical excitation. It is undoubtedly of advantage to the body that the nerve-cells should fatigue before the muscles, for the muscles are thereby pro- tected from the injurious effects of overwork, and are always ready to serve the brain.2 It may be added that nerve-cells not only fatigue more quickly, but recover from fatigue more rapidly than the muscles. (e) Effect of Temperature upon Muscular Contraction. — Heat, within certain limits, increases the irritability and conductivity of muscle-tissue, and at the same time has a favoring influence upon those forms of chemical change which liberate energy. The effect of a rise of temperature, as shown by the myo- gram, is a shortening of the latent period, an increase in the height of contrac- tion, and a quickening of the contraction and relaxation, the whole curve being shortened. Of course there is an upper limit to this favoring action, since, at a c FlG. f>0. — Schema of effect of temperature on height and form of contraction curve : a, contraction at 19° C. ; b, c, d, e,f, contractions made at intervals, each one at a lower temperature; g, h, contractions at higher temperatures than 19° C, h being made when the temperature was 30° C. ; i, k, I, show a different series of contractions, made as the temperature was increased from 30° C. toward the point at which the muscle-substance coagulates (after Gad and Heymans). certain temperature, about 45° C. for frog's muscle and about 50° C. for the striated muscles of warm-blooded animals, 53°— 58° C. for the non-striated muscles of the bladder of the cat,3 heat-rigor begins, and this change is accom- panied by a loss of all vital properties. Cold can be said, in general, to pro- duce effects the opposite of those of heat; as the muscle is cooled, the latent period, the contraction, and the relaxation are all prolonged. Nevertheless, the effect of temperature is not a simple one (see Fig. 60). If during the cooling process a striated muscle of a frog be irritated from time to time with single induction shocks, the height of the contractions does not con- tinually grow less as one would expect.* The maximal height is obtained at 30° C, the height above this point being somewhat less, the irritability les- sening as the coagulation-point is approached; from 30° C. to 19° C. the height continually decreases, but from 19° to 0° C. the height increases, while 'Lombard: Archives italiennes de Biologie, xiii. p. 1 ; or American Journal of Psychology, 1890, p. 1 ; Journal of Physiology, 1892, p. 1 ; 1893, p. 97. 'Waller: Brain, 1891, p. 179. SC. C. Stewart- American Journal of Physiology, 1900, iii. p. 25. '•lad und Heymans: Arehivfur Anatomic wnd Physiologic, 1890, S. 73. GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. 137 below 0° 0. it again becomes less, until at the freezing-point of muscle no con- traction is obtained. The cause of these peculiar phenomena is not definitely understood. (/) Effect of Drugs and Chemicals upon Muscular Contraction. — Certain drugs and chemicals have a marked effect upon the irritability (see p. 58) and con- ductivity (see p. 93) of muscles, and these effects must necessarily find expres- sion in the amount of contraction which would be excited by a given irri- tant. In addition to this, it is worthy of notice that the character of the con- traction may be altered. The drug which has the most striking effect upon the form of contraction is veratria. A few drops of a 1 per cent, solution of the acetate of veratria, injected into the dorsal lymph sac of a frog whose brain has first been destroyed, in a few minutes alter completely the character of the reflex movements : the muscles are still capable of rapidly contracting, but the con- tractions are cramp-like, the power to relax being greatly lessened. The poison acts upon the muscle-substance, and even a very small dose applied Fig. 61.— Myogram of muscle poisoned with veratria and that of a normal muscle : a, myogram from a normal gastrocnemius muscle of a frog— the waves at the close are due to the recoil of the recording lever; b, myogram from a gastrocnemius muscle poisoned with veratria. recorded at the same part of the drum. directly to the muscle for a few hours — e. g., a solution containing 1 part to 100,000 of 0.6 per cent, solution of sodium chloride — suffices to greatly alter the character of the contraction called out by various irritants.1 If a muscle poisoned with veratria be isolated and connected with a myograph, a contrac- tion excited by a single induction shock will show a rise as rapid, as high, and as strong as normal, but the fall of the curve will be greatly prolonged (see Fig. 61). Often the crest of the curve will exhibit a notch, which shows that relaxation may begin and be checked by a second contraction process which carries the curve up again and holds it there for a considerable time. In the above experiment the contracture effect followed the primary contrac- tion immediately. The nature of the contracture of a muscle poisoned with veratria has been considered (sec p. 130). There arc a number of drugs which have an action on muscle-tissue simi- lar to that of veratria — e.g., cornutinc2 produces a similar effect on striated muscles; digitaline increases the tonus of hearl muscle and of the smooth 'Bucannan: Journal of Physiology, 1899, xxv. p. 137. * Cushny : Pharmacology and Therapeutics, 1899. 138 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. muscle-tissue of the walls of the blood-vessels ; epinephrin,1 the active prin- ciple of the extracts obtained from the medullary part of the suprarenal capsules, may be mentioned here, and is of especial interest because derived from the animal body. If injected into the blood, it increases the strength and prolongs the contraction of the muscles generally, and causes through its effect on the muscle of* the heart and the non-striated muscles of the blood- vessels a marked rise of blood-pressure.2 Barium salts, and to a less degree calcium and strontium, act similarly to veratria to prolong the relaxation of the muscle, without lessening the rapidity and height of contraction. Potassium and ammonium salts and a large number of other chemical substances and drugs act to kill the muscle, and as the death process develops excitation produces prolonged localized contractions. This etfect seems to be quite different from that of veratria, for it is accompanied by a rapid lessen- ing of the muscular power. 5. Liberation of Energy by the Contracting- Muscle. — The law of con- servation of energy applies no less to the living body than to the inanimate world in which it dwells. Every manifestation of life is the result of the liberation of energy which was stored in the body in the form of chemical compounds. When a muscle is excited to action it undergoes chemical changes, which are accompanied by the conversion of potential into kinetic en- ergy. This active energy leaves the muscle in part as thermal energy, in part as mechanical energy, and, to a slight extent, under certain conditions, as elec- trical energy. In general, the sum of the liberated energy is given off as heat or motion. The proportion in which these two forms of energy shall be pro- duced by a muscle may vary within wide limits, according to the state of the muscle and the conditions under which the work is done. Fick 3 states that if the muscle works against a very heavy weight, possibly one-fourth of the liberated energy may be obtained as mechanical work; but if the weight be light not more than one-twentieth of the chemical energy is given off in this form, the muscle working no more economically than a steam engine. Zuntz4 studied the work that the body as a whole could accomplish, and found that somewhat more than one-third of the energy liberated can be obtained as external mechanical work. The fact that always a part, and often the whole, of the mechanical energy developed by the muscle is converted to thermal energy within the muscle, and leaves it as heat, makes it the more difficult to determine in what proportion these two forms of energy were originally pro- duced. Moreover, if Engelmann's view be correct, that the change of form exhibited by the muscle is the result of the imbibition of the fluid of the isotropic substance by the anisotropic material, this change being brought about by the heat which is liberated within the muscle, we must consider potential energy to be set free first as heat, a part of which is afterward 1 AU'l : Zeit8chrift fur phyxiologische Chemie, 1899, Bd. xxviii. S. 354. '-'< Hivcr and Schafer: Journal of Physiology, 1895, xviii. pp. 230-276. 3 Fick : Pfliiger'a Archiv, 1878, xvi. S. 85. 4 Tbid., 1897. Bd. Ixviii., 8. 191. GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. 139 changed to mechanical energy, which in part, at least, is again changed to heat. Liberation of Mechanical Energy. — The amount of work which a muscle can do depends on the following conditions : (a) The kind of muscle. The muscles of warm-blooded animals are stronger than those of cold-blooded animals ; a human muscle can do twice the amount of work of an equal amount of frog's muscle. The muscles of certain insects have even greater strength.1 Within the same animal there are great differ- ences in the capacity of different forms of muscle tissue (see p. 107). Pale striated muscle tissue, although more capable of rapid liberation of energy, has not the endurance or the strength of the red striated muscle tissue ; and different forms of non-striated muscle differ among themselves as well as from striated in their capacity for work. (6) The condition of the muscle. Any of the influences which lessen the irritability of the muscle — lack of blood, fatigue, cold, etc. — decreases the power to liberate energy, and any influence which heightens the irritability is favora- ble to the work. The effect of tension to heighten irritability has already been referred to and is of especial interest in this connection, since the very re- sistance of the weight is, within limits, a condition favorable to the liberation of the energy required to overcome the resistance. This will be referred to again. (c) The strength and character of the stimulus. The liberation of energy is, up to a certain point, the greater, the stronger the excitation. Furthermore, rapidly repeated excitations are much more effective than single excitations, because a series of rapidly following stimuli, both by altering the irritability and by inducing the form of contraction known as tetanus, act to produce powerful and high contractions. Bernstein states that the energy developed by the muscle increases with the increase of the rate of excitation from 10 to 50 per second, at which rate the contraction power may be double that called out by a single excitation. (d) T7ie method of contraction and the mechanical conditio)is under winch the work is done. In estimating the amount of mechanical energy liberated by a muscle, we observe the amount of external work which it accomplishes, i. e. the amount of mechanical energy which it imparts to external objects. If a muscle by contracting raises a weight, it gives energy to the weight, the amount being exactly that which the weight in falling through the distance which it was raised by the muscle can impart as motion, heat, etc., to the objects with which it conies in contact. The measure of the mechanical work done by the contracting muscle is the product of the weight into the height to which it is lifted. For example, if a muscle raises a weight of 5 grams 10 millimeters, it does 50 grammillimeters of work. An unweighted muscle in contracting does no external work; a muscle, however vigorously it may contract, if it be prevented from shortening, does no external work ; finally, a muscle which raises a weight and then lowers it again when it relaxes, does not alter its surroundings as the total result of its activity, and 1 Hermann : Handbuch der Physiologie, 1879, Bd. i. S. 64. 140 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. hence does no external work. Although no external work is accomplished under these circumstances, internal work is being done, as is evidenced by the heat evolved by the muscle and the fatigue produced. Unquestionably mechanical energy is developed within the muscle in all these cases, but it is all converted to heat before it leaves the muscle. The amount of weight is an important factor in determining the extent to which a muscle will shorten when excited by a given stimulus, and, therefore, the quantity of work which it will accomplish. If a muscle be after-loaded, i. e. if the weight be supported at the normal resting length of the muscle, and the muscle be excited to a series of maximal contractions, the weight being in- creased to a like amount before each of the succeeding excitations, there is, in general, a gradual lessening in the height of the contractions, but the de- crease in height is not proportional to the increase of the weight. The decrease in the height of contractions is, as a rule, more rapid at the beginning of the series than later, though at times an opposite tendency may show itself and the increasing weights temporarily increase the irritability and therefore increase the amount of shortening. The effect of tension to increase the activ- ity of the contraction process is seen if a muscle which is connected with a strong spring or heavy weight be excited to isometric contractions and in the midst of a contraction be suddenly released ; the muscle under such cir- cumstances is found to contract higher than when excited by the same stimulus without being subjected to tension.1 The effect of tension on the activity of muscular contractions is to be clearly seen iu the case of the heart muscle. A l-ist' of pressure of the fluid within the isolated heart of a frog increases the strength as well as the rate of the beat. If the weight be gradually increased, although the height of the contrac- tions is lessened, the work will for a time increase, and a curve of work (con- structed by raising ordinates of a length corresponding to the work done, from points on an abscissa at distances proportional to the weights em- ployed), will be seen to rise. After the weight has been increased to a cer- tain amount the decline in the height of contractions will be so great that the product of the weight into the height will begin to decrease, and the curve of work will fall, until finally a weight will be reached which the contracting muscle can just support at, but not raise above, its normal resting length This weight will be a measure of the absolute muscular force. Example. Load Height of lift Work (grams). (millimeters). (grammillimeters). 0 13 0 30 11 330 60 9 540 90 7 630 120 5 600 150 3 450 180 2 '. . 360 210 0 0 1 Fick : Mechanische A rbeit, etc., S. 132. Santesson : Skandinavisches Archiv fiir Physiologic, 1889, i. S. 56. GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OE MUSCLE AND NERVE. 141 In the above experiment 30 grams were added to the muscle after each contraction ; as the weight was increased up to 90 grams the amount of work was increased, with greater weights the amount of work was lessened. It is evident that the absolute force of a muscle of a given type will depend not only on the quantity, but also on the arrangement of the microscopic ele- ments of which the muscle is composed. Each element of a fibre lias to stand the strain of the whole fibre ; so the force to be developed depends not on the length of the fibres, but on the number of muscle elements which are arranged side by side, i. e. the absolute force of a muscle will be proportionate to the number of fibres. This can be stated for a muscle with parallel fibres in terms of the cross section of the muscle. In the case of a muscle like the gastrocnemius, where the fibres take an oblique course and are inserted into a common tendon in the middle, the " physiological cross-section " has to be estimated, i. e. the total section taken at right angles to the fibres. Such a muscle is very strong in proportion to its thickness. Rosenthal estimated the absolute force of striated muscles of the frog to be about 3 kilograms per square centimeter, and Hermann l found the absolute force of striated muscle of man to be 6.24 kilograms per square centimeter. The physiological work of which a muscle is capable, on the other hand, is dependent not only on the weight which it can lift, but also the height to which the weight can be lifted. All the muscle elements, whether arranged side by side or in chains, influence the result, and for purposes of comparison one can state the capacity of the muscle for work in terms of the unit of bulk, the cubic centimeter, or the unit of weight, the gram. Thus, Fick states the maximal amount of external work of which frog's muscle is capable, as one grammeter per gram of muscle substance. From what has been said it is evident that the amount of muscle sub- stance determines the amount of work of which the muscle is capable, while the arrangement of the muscle substance decides the character of the work which it is best fitted to perform. Muscles with long parallel fibres, even though of small sectional area, such as the sartorius, are specially fitted to produce extensive movements of the parts to which they are attached ; and muscles which have a large number of fibres, even though these be short, as in the case of the gastrocnemius, are adapted to move great weights. Carvallo and Weiss2 state that the gastrocnemius muscle of the frog when at rest tears if subjected to a weight of 2 kilos. Its contraction power is estimated to be half a kilo, and when it is contracting its resistance is cor- respondingly increased, so that a weight of 2£ kilos is required to rupture it. The increased resistance can be best explained on the idea that, as Prliiger thinks, a new chemical attraction force is developed in contraction. Liberation of Thermal Energy. — Energy leaves the body as mechanical energy only when by its movements the body imparts energy to surrounding objects. Most of the energy liberated within the body leaves it as heat; 1 Pfluger's Archiv, 1898, Bd. 73, S. 429. 1 Carvallo and Weiss: Comptes rendus Societe de Biologic, 1899, p. 122. 142 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. even during violent muscular exercise five times more energy may be ex- pended as heat than as mechanical energy, and the disproportion may be even oreater than this. Rosenthal says that at the most not more than 30 per cent, of the energy developed in the muscle by oxidation and splitting processes is to be got as available mechanical energy. So great is the pro- duction of heat during exercise that, in spite of the great amount leaving the body, the temperature of an oarsman has been found to be increased, during a race of 2000 meters, from 37.5° C. to 39° or 40° C.1 It is exceedingly difficult to ascertain with accuracy on the warm-blooded animal the exact relation of heat-produceion to muscular contraction. The best results have been obtained by experiments on isolated muscles of cold- blooded animals. Helmholtz observed the temperature of a muscle of a frog to be increased by tetanus lasting a couple of minutes 0.14° to 0.18° C. ; Heidenhain saw a change of 0.005° C. result from a single contraction; and Fick ascertained that a fresh, isolated muscle of a frog can by a single contraction produce per gram of muscle-substance enough heat to raise 3 milligrams of water 1° C.2 To obtain evidence of the slight changes of temperature which occur in such small masses of muscle-tissue it is necessary to employ a very delicate instrument, such as a thermopile or a bolometer. TJie thermopile consists of strips of two dissimilar metals, united at their extremities, so as to form a series of thermo-electric junctions. If there be a difference of temperature at two such junctions, a difference of electric potential is developed, which causes the flow of an electric current. If the current be passed through the coils of wire of a galvanometer its amount can be measured, and the extent of the change in tempera- ture at one of the junctions, the other remaining constant, can be estimated. In the more sensitive instruments, several thermo-electric junctions are used. The amount of current depends largely on the metals employed, antimony and bismuth being a very sensitive combination. The action of the bolometer is based on the fact that the resistance of a wire to the passage of an electric current changes with its temperature. The amount of heat developed within the muscle by direct conversion of potential to thermal energy, and the amount formed indirectly, through con- version of mechanical to thermal energy, has been made a subject of careful study by Heidenhain,3 Fick and his pupils,4 and others, the experiments being made chiefly with isolated muscles of frogs. In general, the stronger the stimulus and the greater the irritability of the muscle — in other words, the more extensive the chemical changes excited in the muscle — the greater the amount, not only of mechanical, but of thermal energy liberated. Increase of tension, which is very favorable to muscular 1 George Kolb : Physiology of Sport, translated from the German, second edition, London. 1892. J Fick : Pfliiger'a Archiv, 1878, xvi. S. 89. 3 Michanische Leistung, Warmeentwicklung und Stoffumsatz bei der Muskdthatigkeit, Leipzig, 1864. 4 Mnothermische Untersuchungen aus den physiologischen Laboratorium zu Zurich and Wurzburg, "Wiesbaden, 1889. GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. 143 activity, greatly increases the heat-production. For this reason isometric contractions, that is, those in which the muscle works against a resistance which is sufficient to prevent it from shortening, are accompanied by a greater liberation of heat than isotonic contractions, in which the contracting muscle raises a constant weight. As the weight is increased, both the amount of heat developed and the work are increased, but the liberation of heat reaches its maximum and begins to decline sooner than the amount of work — i.e., with large weights the muscle works more economically; simi- larly, as the muscle is weakened by fatigue the heat-production lessens sooner than the work. Muscle-tonus and Chemical Tonus. — During waking hours, the cells of the central nervous system are continually under the influence of a shower of weak nervous impulses, coming from the sensory organs all over the body ; l moreover, activity of brain-cells, especially emotional forms of activity, leads to an overflow of nervous impulses to the spinal cord and an increased irrita- bility, or, if stronger, excitation of motor nerve-cells. If, when one is quietly sitting and reading, he turns his attention to the sensory impressions which are coming at every moment from all over the body to the brain, notes the temperature of different parts of the skin, the pressure of the clothes, etc., upon different parts, the light reflected from neighboring objects, and the slight sounds about him, he will recognize that the central nervous system is all the time subject to a vast number of excitations, which, because of their very repetition, are ordinarily disregarded by the mind, but which are, nevertheless, all the time influencing the nerve-cells. The efl'ect of this multitude of affer- ent stimuli, in spite of their feebleness, is to cause the motor cells of the curd to continually send delicate motor stimuli to the muscles. These cause the muscle to keep in the state of slight but continued contraction which gives the tension peculiar to waking hours, and which is called muscle-tonus. That such a tension exists is made evident by the change in attitude which occurs when the relaxation accompanying sleep comes on. The effect of brain activ- ity to cause muscular tension is, likewise, most easily recognized by observing the relaxation of the muscles which occurs when mental excitement ceases. Muscle-tonus, like every form of muscular contraction, is the result of chem- ical change, and the liberation of energy. But little of this energy leaves the body as mechanical energy, most of it being given off as heat. This view is by no means universally accepted, and many physiologists believe in a production of heat by the muscles, as a result of nervous influences, independent of contraction. It is thought that a condition of slight but con- tinuous chemical activity resulting in the production, of heat may lie maintained in the muscles by intermittent but frequent reflex excitations, a condition which has been called chemical tonus.2 Thai the chemical activity of muscles is kept 1 Rrondgeest: Archiv fur Anatomie und Physiologie, 1860, S. 703; Hermann, Ibid., L861, S. 350. 2 Koehrig und Zuntz: Pfliiger'a Archie, 1871, lid. iv ; Piliiger: Pjluger'a Archie, 1878, xviii. S. 247. 144 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. up by small stimuli from the spinal cord is shown by the fact that if the nerves be severed, or the nerve-ends be poisoned by curare, the muscle absorbs less oxygen and gives off less carbon dioxide than when at rest under normal conditions.1 The theory of a reflex chemical tonus independent of contraction implies the existence of special nervous mechanisms for the exciting of chemical changes in the muscles which shall result in the liberation of energy as heat, independent of the change of form of the muscle. The question of the exist- ence of special nervous mechanisms controlling heat-production — heat-centres, as they are called — will be considered in another part of this book. E. Electrical Phenomena in Muscle and Nerve.2 The active muscle liberates three forms of energy : mechanical work, heat, and electricity. The active nerve makes no visible movements, gives off no recognizable quantity of heat, but exhibits changes in electrical condition quite comparable to those observed in the active muscle. The electrical changes in nerves are the only evidence of activity which we can observe, aside from the effect of the nerve on the organ which it excites ; they are there- fore of great interest to us. ' Electrical energy, like all forms of active energy, is the result of a trans- formation of potential or some form of kinetic energy. In the case of the muscle, as of an electric battery, we find electricity to be associated with chemical change, and believe it to be liberated from stored potential energy. In the case of nerves no chemical change can be detected during action, and hence we are at a loss to explain the development of electricity. We can only say that it is the result of some chemical or physical process which we have as yet failed to discover. Although activity of nerve and muscle is found to be associated with electrical change, we must not suppose functional activity to be in any sense an electrical process. The movements 1 Zimtz: Pjlii'in-'s Archiv, 1876, xii. 522; Colasanti, Ibid., 1878, xvi. S. 57. 3 Biedermann : Electrophysiologic, Jena, 1895, Bd. ii.; translation by F. A. Wei by, 1898; Waller: Lectures on Animal Electricity, London, 1897. Fig. 02.— Schema of galvanometer: n, .s, north and south poles >>f astatic pair of magnets; m, compensating magnet, held by friction on the staff, and capable of being approached to, or ro- tated with reference to, the suspended magnet ; , mirror ;/, fibre supporting the magnets; e,c, c, c, coils of wire to carry the electric current near to the magnets, the upper coils being wound in the opposite direction to the lower; e, i , imn polarizablc electrodes applied to the longitudinal surface and cross section of a muscle. GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. 145 of a man may be interpreted from the movements of his shadow, but they are very different phenomena; the activity of the nerve and muscle is indi- cated by the electrical changes accompanying it, but they may be independent processes. Certainly the irritating change which is transmitted along the nerve and which excites the muscle to action, although accompanied by elec- trical changes, is not itself an electric current. Electrical energy is exhibited not only by active nerve and muscle, but during the activity of a great variety of forms of living matter. It may be detected in gland-cells, in the cells of many of the lower animal organisms, and even in plant-cells. The amount of electrical energy developed in animal tissues may be far from trivial. Although delicate instruments are necessary to observe the electrical changes in nerve and muscle, as the great internal resistance of the tissues causes the currents to be small, we find in certain fish special electric organs, which appear to be modified muscle tissue, and which are capable of discharging a great amount of electrical energy when excited through their nerves. So intense is the action of this electrical apparatus that it can be used as a weapon of defence and offence. Got eh and Burch state that the electric organ of the malapterurus electricus can give a shock having an electric potential of 200 volts.1 1. Methods of Ascertaining the Electrical Condition of a Muscle or a Nerve. — If the electric tension of any two parts of an object differs, the instant they are joined an electric current will flow from the point where the tension is greater to that where it is less. The presence, direction of flow, and strength of an electric current can be detected by an instrument called a galvanometer. If any two parts of a muscle or nerve, as e, e, Figure 62, be connected by suitable conductors with the coils, c, c, of a galvanometer, and if there be a difference in the electric potential of the two parts examined, an electric current will be indicated by the instrument. In such tests all extra sources of electricity are to be avoided, therefore the electrodes applied to the muscle must be non-polarizable. The Giilriiitu/iu fer — An ordinary form of galvanometer consists of a magnet suspended by an exceedingly delicate fibre of silk, or quartz, and one or more coils, composed of many windings of pure copper wire, placed vertically near the magnet and in the plane of the mag- netic meridian. If an electric current be allowed to flow through the wire, it influences the magnetic field about it, and, if the coils he close to the suspended magnet, causes the magnet to deviate from the plane of the magnetic meridian in one or the other direction, according to the direction of the flow of the current. In the more delicate instruments the influence of the earths magnetism is lessened by the use of two magnets of as nearly as pos sible the same Strength, placed so as to point in opposite directions, and fastened at the extremities of a light rod. As each magnet tends to point toward the north, they mutually Oppose each other, and therefore I lie effect of I lie earth's magnetism is partly compensated. Still another magnet may he brought near this " astatic " combination, and by opposing the action of the earth's magnetism make the arrangement even more delicate. In the Thomp- son galvanometer, the rod connecting the needles hears a slightly concave mirror, from which a beam of lighl can lie reflected on a scale. Or a scale may be placed so that iis image falls on the mirror, and the slightest movement of the magnet may be read in the mirror by a telescope. The ordinary galvanometer is influenced by changes in the magnetism of the earth. and by earth currents which may he due to an escape of electricity from neighboring street-car circuits, etc. These disturbances may interfere with the use of the instrument, 1 Proceedings of the Royal Society, 1900, vol. lxv. p. I 12. Vol. ir.— in 146 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. because they may lead to uncontrollable movements of the magnet, and a consequent shift- ing of the 0 point. There arc other forms of instruments, such as the Deprez-d' Arson val mirror galvanometer,1 which arc not affected by Buch influences. The galvanometer i- very sensitive to the presence of electric currents. Another appa- ratus which is even more responsive to changes in electric potential of short duration is tli*' capillary electrometer. The capillary electrometer (Fig. 63) consists of a glass tube (a) drawn out to form a very fine capillary, the end of which dips into a glass cup with parallel sides (/) contain- ing a l<> per cent, solution of sulphuric acid. The upper part of the tube is connected by a thick- walled rubber tube with a pressure-bulb containing mercury (<■). As the pressure-bulb is raised, the mercury is driven into the capillary, the flow being opposed by the capillary resistance. By a suffi- ciently great pressure, mercury may he driven to the extremity of the capillary and all the air expelled. When the pressure is relieved the mercury rises again in the tube, drawing the sulphuric acid after it. The column of mercury will come to rest at a point where the pressure and the capillary force just balance. Seen through the microscope (e), the end of the column of mercury, where it is in contact with the sulphuric acid appears as a convex menis- cus ( thai caused by the injury. The diminution of the current of injury, which was less than in some oilier experiments, was onus volt. The time record at the bottom of the curve was obtained from a tuning fork making 500 double vibrations perse id afb r Burdon San- derson). tional effect" is only to be observed upon an injured muscle, since it repre- sents a difference in potential between the normally contract ing and the injured, imperfectly contracting muscle-substance. When all parts of the muscle are normal and contracting to an equal amount, the electrical force- would be everywhere of the same nature, balance one another, and give no external evidence. Although the diminutional effect is only to be observed upon the injured muscle, the temporary negative changes which follow each excitation 1 Journal of Physiology, 1895, vol. xviii. p. 717. 152 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. are to be observed on the normal muscle. To understand this we must con- sider the diphasic current of action. Diphasic Owrr&rd of Actum. — If a normal muscle be locally stimulated by a single irritation, either directly or indirectly through its nerve, the part excited will be the first to become active and electrically negative, and this condition will be taken on later by other parts. Our methods only permit us to observe the relative condition of the parts of the muscle to which the elec- trodes are applied, the changes in the intermediate tissue failing to show theru- selves. If an electrode be applied near the place where the uninjured muscle is stimulated. .1, and another at some distant point, B, and these electrodes be connected with a capillary electrometer, a diphasic electrical change will be observed to follow each stimulation. At the instant the irritant is applied the muscle-substance at A will become suddenly negative with respect to that at B ; when the spreading irritation wave has reached B, that part too will tend to be negative, and an electrical equality will be temporarily established; finally, B continuing to be active after A has ceased to act, B will be negative in respect to A. Since the wave of excitation spreads along the fibres in both directions from the point irritated, each excitation will cause diphasic electrical changes to either side of the place to which the irritant is applied. lt^ the muscle has been injured at B, the dying fibres there will react but poorly to the stimulus, and therefore the antagonistic influence of the negative change at B will incompletely compensate for the negativity at A, and hence only a >ingle phase due to the condition of negativity at A will be seen. The normally beating heart ventricle shows diphasic currents of action : in the first phase the base, where the contraction process start.-, is negative to the apex, and in the second phase the apex is negative to the base. In ease the heart be injured, the negative change corresponding to action fails at the injured part, and therefore a single and because not antagonized more pro- longed negative change is observed. Under certain conditions a triphasic change is observed, which need not be discussed here. Waller1 has succeeded in recording the electrical changes which accompany the beat of the human heart. These diphasic changes of the electric condition are sufficiently strong and rapid in the mammalian heart to excite the nerve of a nerve-muscle prepara- tion, and the muscle will lie seen to give one, or, if the heart is uninjured, sometimes two, contractions avevy time the heart beats. Bernstein2 found the time between the two portions of diphasic change to be proportional to the distance between the leading-off electrodes, and to cor- respond to a rate of transmission the same as that of the wave of excitation, as revealed by the spread of the contraction process (in the muscle of the frog 3 meters per second). Hermann,3 by using cord electrodes on the human fore- 1 Archiv fur Anatomu und Physiologie, 1890; physiol. Abtheil., S. 187. 2 Untersuch a 1,,/,-n iiber den Erregungsvorgang im Nerven- mul Mu.l<>(fie, 1SS7, S. 'J04. 154 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. be injured, and the normal surface l>c compared with the dying or dead cross section, the second phase is absent. If the nerve be frequently excited, each excitation awakens a separate currenl of action. Although nerves are excited most readily by electric currents, negative variations of the demarcation current maybe called out by various chemicals — e.g., salt or glycerin ' — and by mechanical excitation, such as a sharp cut with the shears.2 It is a physiological phenomenon, for a negative change may be observed to accompany a uerve impulse which has been caused by the spread of excitation from central neurones along their peripheral axones. Du Bois-Reymond observed with the galvanometer a lessening (''negative variation ") of the demarcation current (" current of rest ") when in strychnia- poisoning the spinal motor neurones were sending vigorous impulses along their axones and causing cramp-like tetanic muscular contractions. Gotch and I Iorslev ; applied electrodes connected with a capillary electrometer to peripheral nerves, spinal nerve-roots, and tracts of motor fibres within the spinal cord, and discovered that if the cortical brain-cells in the motor zones were excited, the nerves showed currents of action corresponding in rate to the discharge of motor impulses from these brain-cells — e.g., if the epilepti- form convulsions were occurring at the time, the capillary electrometer revealed changes of potential of like rate in the nerves. Macdonald and Rwd ' observed currents of action in the phrenic nerve of mammals which corresponded in time with the respiratory movements. These were due to the normal discharge of nerve impulses from the respira- tory centers. When a condition of apnoea was established and the respiratory movements ceased, the electrical change failed to appear; when the respiratory movements were quickened in dyspnoea, the rhythmic movements of the gal- vanometer were quickened to correspond ; when during asphyxia the respi- rations were of the Cheyne-Stokes type, the currents of action showed a like rhythm. Even physiological sensory nerve-impulses have been found to produce negative variation currents. Stcinach5 observed currents of action to be caused by mechanical pressure on the skin of the frog. If the pressure was continued, the negative change gradually decreased, and a new negative variation was seen if the pressure was suddenly removed. Light falling on the retina of the eye of a frog has been seen to cause a negative variation of the current of rest of the optic nerve. The electrical change which we call the current of action can be thought to sweep over the nerve as a wave, having in the inedullated nerves of the frog a length of IS mm., and travelling at the rate of 28 meters per second. The duration of the negative variation is different in different kinds of nerves 1 Kiihne unn .\>iims Archiv, L894-1897, Bd. Iviii., lix., lxiii., lxv., lxvi., Ixix. • Lectures on Animal Electricity, London, 1897. 4 Boruttau: Pfliiger'a Archiv, 1894, lid. Iviii. S. 64. 5 Biedermann : I^lectrophysioloyy, translated by F. A. VVelby, 1898, vol. ii. p. 303. GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. 159 made. Purely physiological phenomena are such as can be exhibited only by a mechanism which has the chemical and physical structure of living protoplasm, and such as cease with the life of the protoplasm. The electrical phenomena of nerve are capable of being divided into two classes, the one, purely chemico-physical, resulting from the core-conductor- like structure, and the other, physiological, intimately dependent on the reac- tions of the living protoplasm. The medullated nerve is not merely a core- conductor. It is too soon to try to separate these two classes of phenomena ; we must wait not only for more work to be done on nerves, but on other irritable forms of protoplasm, for many of these, although of entirely different structure from the nerve, exhibit very similar electrical reactions. F. Chemistry of Muscle and Nerve. I. Chemistry of Muscle. Muscles consist of muscle-fibres bound together by connective tissue. Between the fibres we find nerves, blood-vessels, and lymphatics. Fat-cells containing considerable fat may also be found in the midst of the connective- tissue network. Each fibre consists of a sheath, the sarcolemma, which resembles elastin in its constitution, and within this the muscle-substance proper, together with certain substances of nutritive value and waste prod- ucts. Muscle which has been freed as far as possible from blood, connective tissue, and fat, has a mean specific gravity of 1.060; the extreme variations found for the muscles of different animals being 1.053-1. 074. l When it is fresh the reaction is slightly alkaline. It contains about 75 parts of water and 25 parts of solids; nearly '_'<> parts of the solids are proteids, the remaining 5 parts consisting of fats, ex- tractives, and salts. Little is known concerning the chemistry of living muscle; the instability of the complex molecules which make- possible the rapid development of energy peculiar to muscles renders exact analysis impossible. The manipulations essential to chemical analysis necessarily alter and kill the muscle protoplasm. Death of the muscle is ordinarily associated with a peculiar chemical change known as rigor mortis. To understand the chemical composition of muscle it is necessary that we should consider the nature of this change. 1. Rigor Mortis. — Rigor mortis, the rigidity of death, is the result of a chemical change in the substance of a muscle by which it is permanently altered, its irritability and other vital properties being irretrievably lost. The change is manifested bya loss of translucency, the muscle becoming opaque, and by a gradual contraction, accompanied by a development of heal and acidity, and resulting in the muscle being stiff and firm to the touch, less elastic, and less extensible. Whenever muscle dies it undergoes this change. M'arvallo and Weiss: Journal dc I'lu/sinlixjir, ls'.i'.i, i. p. 1204. 160 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. Conditions which Influence the Development of Rigor. — Ordinarily on the deatli of the body the muscle enters into rigor slowly — the muscle-fibres are involved one after the other, and through the gradual contraction and harden- ing of the antagonistic muscles the joints become fixed and the body acquires the rigidity which we associate with death. Rigor usually affects the different parts of the body in a regular order, from above downward, the jaw, neck, trunk, arms, and legs being influenced one after the other. The position taken by the body is generally determined by the weight of the parts and the rela- tive strength of the contractions of the muscles. The time required for the appearance of rigor is very variable. It is deter- mined in part by the nature of the muscle, its condition at the moment of death, and the temperature to which it is subjected. The muscles of warm- blooded animals enter into rigor more quickly than those of cold-blooded animals; of the warm-blooded animals, pale muscles more quickly than red, and the flexors before the extensors ; of the cold-blooded animals, frog's muscles more quickly than those of the turtle. In general, the more active the muscle protoplasm, the more rapid are the chemical changes which it undergoes, and amongst these the coagulation of rigor mortis. The condition of the muscle plays a very important part in determining the onset of rigor. If the muscles are strong and vigorous and death of the body has come suddenly, rigor develops slowly ; if the muscles have been enfeebled by disease or fatigued by great exertion shortly before death, it comes rapidly. In the case of wasting diseases rigor comes quickly, is poorly developed, and passes oil" quickly ; when the muscles are fatigued at the time of death, as in the case of a hunted animal, it comes quickly. We hear of soldiers found dead on the field of battle grasping the sword, as if the muscular contractions of life had been continued by the contractions of death. In the case of certain dis- eases of the spinal cord and brain, too, rigor may come so rapidly that the limbs may maintain the position which they had at the time of death, "cata- leptic rigor," as it has been called. The coming on of rigor is particularly striking in the case of diseases which, like cholera, are accompanied by violent muscular cramps and lead to a rapid death. It is not uncommon, in such cases, for the contractions of rigor to cause movements which may mislead a watcher into supposing the dead man to be still alive. This idea is favored by the fact that the body may remain warm, owing to the heat which is produced in the muscles as a result of the chemical changes occurring during rigor. The post-mortem muscular contractions and the rise of temperature observed in such cases arc only excessive manifestations of what always occurs on the death of the muscle. The movements are probably due, in part, to the rapidity with which the muscles contract in rigor, and in part to the fact that the antagonistic muscles are not affected at the same time to the same degree. Whether the contractions are partly excited by changes accompanying the death of the motor nerve-cells in the central nervous system is uncertain, but Qol impossible. Muscle- are still able to respond by contractions to stimuli coming to them through the nerve, even after rigor has become quite pro- GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. 101 nounced, probably because the coagulation process attacks the different fibres at different rates, and certain of the fibres are still alive and irritable after the others are dead and coagulated. Many observers favor the view that the central nervous system influences muscles after the death of the body as a whole, and by weak stimuli resulting from the changes in the nerve-cells excites chemical changes in the muscles which favor the coming on of rigor.1 In proof of this it is stated that cura- rized muscles enter into rigor more slowly than non-curarized. Undoubtedly stimulation of the nerve, or, indeed, anything which would excite a muscle to action, tends to put it in a condition favorable to the coming on of rigor; whether the influence exerted by the central nervous system is more than this is very questionable. Temperature has a marked influence on the development of rigor mortis. Cold delays and warmth favors, 38°-40° C. being most favorable. Since rigor is the result of a chemical change, these effects of temperature are what one would have expected. Other forms of chemical change which are attributable to ferment action are found to be the most vigorous at a temperature of about 40° C. In general, it may be said that rigor in warm-blooded animals comes on in from ten minutes to seven hours after death, although some state that it may come as late as eighteen hours. It lasts anywhere from one to six days. The sooner it comes on, the sooner it goes off. The stiffness can be broken up artificially by forced movements of the parts, and when thus destroyed does not return, provided the rigor was complete at the time. The Cause and Nature of the Contraction of Rigor Mortis. — The most likely explanation of the contraction of the dying muscle is that it is the result of the coagulation of a part of the semi-fluid muscle-substance within the sarco- lemma. This was suggested by Bruecke, and Kuehne proved that such a coagulation change takes place, by showing that the semi-fluid muscle-sub- stance, " the muscle-plasma," if expressed from the frozen muscle, coagulates on being wanned. The coagulation is the result of a chemical change, by which two proteids of the plasma, paramyosinogen and my osinogen, are converted into the coagulated proteid myosin, this change being produced by the action of a ferment, the myosin ferment, which is thought to be formed at the death of the muscle. Another, though less generally accepted view, is that the contraction of the muscle seen in rigor is of the same nature as ordinary muscular contractions.3 Prolonged muscle contractions are observed under a great variety of condi- tions (see p. 127), and there are many points of resemblance between the contraction of normal and dying muscle — viz., the change of form, the pro- duction of heat, the formation of sarcolactic acid, the using up of oxygen ami the production of carbon dioxide, and the fact that the dying and presumably coagulating muscle is, like normal contracting muscle, electrically negative 1 Brown-Sequard : Archives de Physwlogie, 1889, p. 675. 'Hermann: Handbuch der Phyaiologie, 1879, Bd. i. ■v;. 1 16. Vol. II. ll 162 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. as compared with normal resting muscle. To this may be added that, as has been said, the muscle continues to be irritable even when rigor is quite advanced, and that it enters into rigor more quickly if left in connection with the central nervous system. On the other hand, one cannot fail to be impressed with the differences between the two forms of contraction. Normal Contracting Muscle. Contains uncoagulated proteids. Is translucent. Is soft and flexible. Is no less elastic than in repose. Is more extensible than in repose. Contracts rapidly. Fatigues rapidly and relaxes. Muscle contracting by Rigor Mortis. Contains coagulated myosin. Is opaque. Is firm and still! Is less elastic than before. Is less extensible than before. Contracts very slowly, as a rule. Remains contracted a long time. Furthermore, it may be added that normal contractions only occur when the irritable muscle is stimulated, while a muscle can enter into rigor when its irritability has been taken away by subjecting it to oxalate solutions,1 also, when it has been curarized and so shut out from all nervous influences.2 Rigor is not confined to the voluntary muscles, though it is less easily observed in the case of most involuntary muscles. The heart enters rapidly into rigor, with the formation of sarcolactic acid. The non-striated muscle of the stomach and ureters, too, has been seen to undergo this change. The passing off of rigor mortis is usually accompanied by beginning decomposition, and, indeed, it has been thought that the decomposition is the cause of softening of the muscle. This is denied by certain observers, and it is stated that rigor may pass off when the presence of putrefactive organisms is excluded by special aseptic precautions. Another explanation is that a process of intramuscular digestion goes on. Pepsin, a proteolytic ferment, lias been found in small amounts in the muscle; and acid, which is necessary to the action of this digestive ferment, is formed in the muscle when it coagulates. The presence of these substances would make the diges- tion of proteid material possible, and the fact that proteoses and peptone, products of such digestion, are to be found in the muscle after death, though not present during life, favors the view. It cannot be considered, however, to be definitely established. The rigidity of muscles in rigor can be readily broken up by stretching and massaging the muscles, and when this has been done it does not return. The Chemical Changes which accompany the Development of Rigor. — Rigor mortis is characterized by the coagulation of a part of the muscle-substance; this can be prevented by a temperature a little below 0° C. Cold, although temporarily depriving the muscle of its irritability, does not, unless extreme and long-continued, kill the muscle protoplasm. Frogs can be frozen stiff and recover their activity when they thaw out. Indeed, this probably happens not 1 Howell : Journal of Physiology, 1893, vol. xiv. p. 476. 2 Nagel : l'jlihjrr s Archir, Bd. Iviii. S. 279. GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. 163 infrequently to the frogs hibernating in holes in the banks of ponds. Since cold prevents coagulation without destroying the life of the muscle protoplasm, we can by its aid isolate the living muscle-substance from the nerves, blood- vessels, connective tissue, and sarcolemna of the muscle, but as soon as we begin to analyze it it loses its living structure. This method of obtaining muscle-plasma was introduced by Kuehne1 in the study of the muscles of frogs, and was later employed with slight modifications by Halliburton2 for the mus- cles of warm-blooded animals. The blood was washed out of the vessels with a stream of 0.6 per cent, sodium-chloride solution at 5° C. ; the irritable mus- cles were then quickly cut out and frozen in a mixture of ice and salt. The frozen muscle was then cut up fine in the cold, and a yellowish, some- what viscid, and faintly alkaline muscle-plasma was squeezed out. This fluid was found to coagulate in twenty to thirty minutes at a temperature of 40° C. ; if the temperature were lower the coagulation was slower. The clot, which was jelly-like and translucent, contracted slowly and in a few hours squeezed out a few drops of serum. The coagulated material formed in the clot is called myosin. It dissolves readily in dilute neutral saline solutions, as a 10 per cent, solution of sodium chloride or a 5 per cent, solution of mag- nesium sulphate, and its saline solutions are precipitated in an excess of water or by saturation with sodium chloride, magnesium sulphate, or ammonium sulphate; it has, in short, the characteristics of a globulin. Chittenden and Cummins state that it has the following composition: C 52.82, H 7.11, N 16.17, S 1.27, O 22.03. Halliburton, in studying the coagulation of muscle, followed for the sake of comparison the methods which have been employed in the study of coagulation of blood. He found that muscle-plasma, like blood-plasma, is prevented from coagulating not only by cold, but by neutral salt-, such as magnesium sulphate, sodium chloride, and sodium sulphate; and further, that the salted plasma if diluted coagulates. The points of resemblance between the coagulation of myosin in the muscle and fibrin in the blood suggest a similar cause, and Halliburton suc- ceeded in obtaining from muscles coagulated by long standing in alcohol a watery extract, which greatly hastened the coagulation of muscle-plasma. He called the substance thus obtained myosin ferment. The extract obtained contained an albumose which was either the ferment or held it in close com- bination. The pure ferment has not been isolated. In the case of coagula- tion of the blood, a proteid of the plasma, fibrinogen, is changed by coagula- tion to fibrin, this change being brought about by the action of the fibrin ferment, for the formation of which calcium is necessary. The calcium does not enter into the chemical change independently, and it can go on in the absence of calcium provided the ferment has been already formed. In the case of coagulation of muscle, two proteids of the muscle plasma, paramyosinogen and myosinogen, are changed by coagulation to myosin, or. 1 Untersuchungen fibt r das Protoplasma, Leipzig, 1864. * Journal of Physiology, 1887, vol. viii. pp. 1 33 202. 164 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. as Fiirth says, to myogen-fibrin and niyosin-fibrin ' by the action of myosin ferment. The change can go on in the absence of calcium, but whether this is essential to the formation of the ferment is not yet known. The myosin ferment is not the same as fibrin ferment, since neither can do the work of the other. Moreover, fibrin ferment is destroyed at 75°-80° C. and myosin ferment is not destroyed till 100° C. The chemical change which results in the formation of myosin is different from that which produces fibrin. The clotting of muscle-plasma and the formation of myosins are accompanied or closely followed by the production of an aeid, while no such change occurs during the coagulation of blood-plasma. In the earlier stages of clotting the acidity may be due in part to acid potas- sium phosphate, but the final acidity is chiefly due to lactic acid. The source of the lactic acid has not been definitely made out. The view that it comes from glycogen is made questionable by Bohm's2 observation that the amount of glycogen is not lessened in rigor ; besides, the muscles of starving animals become aeid when entering into l'igor, although, as Bernard found, they con- tain no glycogen. Bohni concluded that the sarcolactic acid may be formed from the proteids. Probably both glycogen and proteids can yield lactic acid. Some writers have thought that the coagulation of the muscle was due to the formation of an acid by the dying muscle. This is unlikely, although the presence of acid, like that of many other substances, quinine, eaffein, digitalin, veratrin, hydrocyanic acid, ether, chloroform, etc., which lead to altera- tions in the conditions of the normal muscle-substance, may hasten the proc- ess. Apparently, anything which causes a deterioration of the muscle-sub- stance, chemical reagents, drugs, or the products of fatiguing work, hastens the coming on of rigor. On the other hand, anything which helps maintain the normal constitution of the muscle appears to postpone the change. Thus Latimer3 reports that the circulation of dextrose through fatigued muscle largely does away with the effect of fatigue to hasten rigor mortis. Nor is this because fatigue products are washed out of the muscle, for the circula- tion of other fluids through the muscle, whether neutral, acid, or alkaline, fails to have the effect. Rigor Caloris. — If a muscle be heated beyond its normal temperature, its irritability is increased, and it undergoes rapid katabolic changes which lead to its death. These changes, if sufficiently rapid, may bring about a con- traction of the muscle, and this contraction, involving the different fibres of the muscle to different degrees, may be continued without break by the con- traction that is peculiar to rigor mortis; in addition to this, if the temperature is raised sufficiently, then' will be a heat precipitation of the various proteids of the muscle, which will lead to a still further shortening, the contraction 1 Faith: Archiv fur experimenteUe Paihologie umd Pharmakologie, 1895, xxvi. 231; and 1896, .xxxvii. 389. Pfluger*8 Archiv, 1880, Bd. xxiii. 8. 44. 8 Latimer: American Journal of Physiology, 1899, ii. p. 29. GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NEBVE. 165 of rigor caloris. The heated muscle may, therefore, be the .seat of three different kinds of processes, each of which leads to a shortening. If frog's muscle be gradually heated, it shows three separate contraction movements at three separate temperatures, at about .'34°, 47°, 58° C. The last two con- tractions are due to heat coagulation of paramyosinogen (myosin of v. Furth) at 47° C, and myosinogen (myogen of v. Furth) at 58° C. These are undoubted effects of heat rigor. There is a difference of opinion as to the nature of the first contraction. It has been generally attributed to the coming on of rigor mortis — i. c, to a post-mortem coagulation of para- myosinogen and myosinogen. In case a muscle be rubbed between the fingers, so that its anatomical condition is altered, although the chemical structure remain the same, this form of shortening does not occur, and it is not until the temperatures at which paramyosinogen and myosinogen are coagulated by heat are reached that the muscle begins to shorten.1 Probably the rigor-mortis change occurs, but on account of the physical change in the fibre it does not reveal itself. This suggests the well-known fact, that when the rigidity of a muscle in rigor has been broken up by mechanical means it does not return. The condition of the muscle has an important influence on the temperature at which it will enter into rigor when heated. Latimer2 reports that a fatigued muscle will go into rigor at a temperature 10 degrees lower than a fresh muscle will. Probably both fatigue and high tempera- ture are favorable to the formation of the myosin ferment, and heat hastens the fermentation process, resulting in coagulation. Another view of the nature of the first form of contraction has been advanced lately. According to Brodie and Richardson,3 this contraction in the case of frog's muscle may be very considerable, and is due to heat coagulation of soluble myogen fibrin, a form of proteid which v. Furth found to be formed from myosinogen (what he termed myogen) at 30° C. Mammalian muscles do not show any marked contraction at this temperature, and have not been found to contain myogen fibrin. This form of shortening is seen only by light loads, for the coagula- tion of the proteids of the muscle causes increased extensibility, in addition to the tendency to contract.1 The change of form in rigor caloris is more in voluntary than in involuntary muscles, as much as 60 per cent, in the former and 10 per cent, in the latter. The beginning of heat-rigor comes at very different temperatures in the different muscles of different animals. Mammalian muscle can stand several degrees higher than the muscles <>f cold-blooded animals, heart-muscle can be heated two or three degrees higher than the skeletal muscles, and skeletal muscles differ, e. g., the semimem- branosus of the frog enters into rigor sooner than the gastrocnemius.8 These facts are brought out in experiments in which the temperature of the muscle Stewart and Sollmann : L899, xxiv. p. 128. 'Latimer: American Journal of Physiology, 1899, ii. p. 20. ■> Philosophical Transactions <>/ the Royal Society <>/ London, 1899, Series B., v.. I. exci. p 363 4 Gotschlich : Journal of Physiology, 1^'.)7, vol. xxi. p. 353. 5 Ward, II. ( '. : Unpublished experiments at tin- University of Michigan. 166 AN AMERICAN TEXT- BO OK OF PHYSIOLOGY. is more or less rapidly raised, at the same time that the changes in the length of the muscle arc recorded on a slowly moving surface. Halliburton1 gives the following precipitation temperatures for muscle proteids. Name. Temperature of coagulation. Proteids obtained from i Paramyosinogen 47° C the dissolved clot . . I Myosinogen 56° C Proteids obtained from f Myoglobin 63° C. muscle-serum ... Myo-albumin 73° C. I Mvo-albumose (not coagulated by heat). Constituents of Muscle-serum. — Proteids. — The fluid which can be expressed from the coagulated fresh muscle is called muscle-plasma, This undergoes a change in the process of coagulation, two of the globulins present, the para -myosinogen and the myosinogen, being precipitated in the form of myosin, which makes the substance of the clot. The fluid which .an be expressed from the clotted muscle, the muscle-serum, therefore lacks at least two of the protcid constituents of the normal muscle. The proteids of the muscle-serum are : myoglobulin, myo-albumin, and mvo-albumose. The myoglobulin resembles serum-globulin, although precipitated at 63° C. instead of 78° C. The myo-albumin is apparently identical with serum- albumin. To these proteids we must add the pigment haemoglobin. Another pig- ment, myolncniatin, is also found. It is not unlikely that these pigments have here as elsewhere a respiratory function. Although the proteids form the larger part of the solids of the muscle- substance, but little is known as to the form in which they exist in the living muscle or the part that they play in its activity. They seem to have a two- fold function, they are at once the machine and the fuel.2 Under normal conditions they probably supply but a small part of the energy set free by the muscle during ordinary work. In excessive muscular work they undergo katabolic change, as is shown by the increased excretion of nitrogen and sul- phur in the urine. In the case of an individual not in training it would appear that during excessive muscular exercise, as in starvation, other parts of the body may give up their proteids to the muscles, for under such cir- cumstances uric acid and phosphorus-holding extractives, the waste products of nuelein, appear in the urine, and the muscle contains but little nuclein.3 This is much less the case if the individual is in training, from which it would appear that through training muscles acquire the capacity of storing more proteid <>r of utilizing their stock to better advantage. In any case if a large amount of muscular work is to be done the amount of proteid in the food should be increased. Nitrogenous Extractives. — The chief nitrogenous extractive is creatin ; in 'Halliburton: Physiological Chemistry, p. 414. 'Pfliiger: Pfluger's Archiv, 1899, lid. lxxvii. S. 425. 3 Dunlop, Paton, Stockman, Maccadam: Journal of Physiology, 1897, xxii. p. 67. GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. 167 addition to this we find small amounts of creatinin and of various xanthin bodies, as xanthin, hypoxanthin, carnin, carnic acid, and sometimes traces of urea, uric acid, taurin, and glycocoll. The chemical nature of these bodies need not be considered here. Physiologically they may be regarded as waste products which result from the partial oxidation of the proteids of muscle during the katabolic processes which are continually occurring even in the resting muscle protoplasm. Monari has shown that the amount of matin and creatinin is increased by the wear and tear of muscular work, although the proteids of the well-fed muscle probably supply but little of the energy which is set free. The non-nitrogenous constituents of muscle are fats, glycogen, inosit, dex- trose, and lactic acid. Fats are usually found in intermuscular connective tissue, and there is some within the normal fibre. It is doubtful whether fat plays a direct part in the ordinary metabolic processes involved in the action of muscles, although it is probable that if more available sources of energy are lacking it may, like the proteids, be altered and employed. Bogdanow l states that the fat which is within the muscle-fibre is of different constitution from that between the fibres ; the extracts contain more free fatty acid. He further found that the fat within the fibre is used up during the work of the muscle, and is con- tinually renewed from the blood. If a muscle of a frog be removed from the circulation and tetanized, it stains much less with osrnic acid than one with its circulation unimpaired; while a muscle which is curarized, and so does no work, if it has a good blood-supply, stains much darker than ordinary muscle. Under pathological conditions large amounts of fat may be found inside the sarcolemma ; in phosphorus-poisoning the degenerated muscle-pro- toplasm may be replaced by fat in the form of fine globules. Glycogen is found in very variable amounts in different muscles. The work of many observers has shown that it is here, as in the liver, a store of carbo- hydrate material, and is employed by the muscle, either directly or after con- version into some other body, as a source of energy. The quantity, which is rarely more than ^ per cent., lessens rapidly during starvation and muscle work. When it is required, it is changed to dextrose, and is finally oxidized to C02 and H..O. If the action of the muscle is prevented by the cutting of the nerve or of the tendon, the glycogen is found to accumulate. Sugar is found in muscles in small quantities only ; nevertheless it probably plays an important part, for ( 'hauveau and Kaufmann, by studying the levator labii superioris of the horse, found that the muscles take sugar from the blood. and that they take more during action than rest. The sugar which the mus- cle takes during rest is for the most part stored as glycogen.-' Although sugar is considered a source of muscle-energy, the exact way in which it is employed is doubtful. 1 Arehiv fur Anatomie vmd Physiologic, 1897, 8 1 l'.'. 2 Qmiptt'x rrnrfus rfc la Sorictr (If Biologic, 1886, civ. 168 AX AMERICAN TEXT- BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. Ergographic experiments on the human subject have proved that muscles which have been fatigued by long-continued voluntary work recover much more rapidly if sugar be eaten. Curiously enough, Waller and Miss Sowton observed that the endurance of an isolated frog's nerve was increased, or at least its capacity to develop strong currents of action was enhanced, if it was put for a time in a <>.<> per cent, solution of sodium chloride containing dex- trose. Lactic Acid. — This is formed in the muscle during work and during coagulation. It has the form of para-lactic acid or sarco-lactic acid, though it i.- doubtful whether it exists in a free state. It is a decomposition product of the carbohydrates and perhaps of proteid or some complex muscle-sub- stance of which proteid forms a part. It is only partly responsible for acidity of the muscle which has been worked. The acidity may well he in pari caused by acid potassium phosphate produced from alkaline phosphates as a result of the formation of phosphoric acid from lecithin, nuelein, etc.1 Rohmann2 attributes the acidity of worked muscles to monopotassium phos- phate, and the alkaline reaction of the resting muscle to dipotassium phos- phate ami sodium bicarbonate. Inorganic Constituents of Muse/,. — Among the bases, potassium has the greatest prominence, and sodium next: magnesium, calcium, and small amounts of iron are also found. Of the acids, phosphoric is present in the largest quan- tities. The quantity of a given substance present in a tissue is not an evidence of its value, and the salts in the muscles, although present in comparatively small quantities, are absolutely essential, not only to their functional activity, but to their life. According to Loeb,3 salts are not only present, as such, in the muscle, but the ions Na, Ca, K, and Mg, are in combination with the proteids, and these ion-proteid compounds are essential to its irritability (see ,,. 58). Gases of Muscle. — Xo free oxygen can be extracted, but carbon dioxide may be obtained, in part free and in part in combination. A little nitrogen can also be extracted, but apparently it has no physiological significance. Tin' amount of carbonic acid developed varies greatly with the condition of the muscle ; for instance, it i- much increased by muscle work. Muscles take up oxygen from the blood freely, especially when active, and when removed from the body may absorb small amounts from the air. Moreover, a certain amount of oxygen comes to the muscle from the food. More oxy- gen is taken up by the muscle during rest than is liberated as carbon dioxide. but during action the reverse is the case.' Oxygen is not retained as free 1 Weyl imil Seither: Zeitschrift fur physiologische Cher/vie, vi. S. ~>~>7. : Rohmann : Pfluger'a Archiv, 1892, 1. S. - 1, and 1893, lv. 589. 3 American Journal <■/' Physiology, 1900, vol. iii. p. 327. *Ludwig mid Sczelkow: Stizungsberichte der I:. Akad. Wien, 1862, Bd, xlv. Abthl. 1; and Ludwig mid Schmidt : Sitzungsberichte '/<•>• mnih.-phys. Clasxe d. k. Sachs. GeseUschafi ■ Wissen- schaft, 1868, Bd. xx.; Regnault and Reisel : Annates d>- Ghimie et de Physique, 1849, 3tne s6r., xxvi.; Pfliiger: Pfliiger's Archiv, 1>7"_\ vi. : and others. GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. 169 oxygen, but is stored in some combination more stable than oxyhemoglobin. It is by virtue of the combined oxygen that the muscle is enabled to do its work, but the process is not one of simple oxidation. That muscles hold oxygen in available combinations was shown by Hermann, who ascertained that a muscle can contract hundreds of times in an atmosphere Tree from oxygen, and produce water and carbon dioxide. If a muscle be thus fatigued, it will recover somewhat in case it be supplied with oxygen, but not otherwise (Joteyko et Riehct). Zuntz l found that the amount of oxygen absorbed by the body during muscular work gives a proportional measure of the energy expended. He gives the following figures for bicycle-riding: Rate per hour. Oxygen absorption per meter. 9 kilometers 4.5 cu. cm. I.". " 4.8 " 21.5 " 5.76 " A comparison of bicycling and walking showed that by moderate speeds (riding 15 kilometers and walking 6 kilometers per hour) about double the amount of oxygen was used for like distances in walking, but about 22 per cent, more was required during like periods of time in riding. II. Chemistry of Nerves. Most of our ideas concerning the chemistry of nerves are based on analysis of the white and gray matter of the central nervous system. The white matter is largely made up of nerve-fibres and supporting tissue, and the gray matter of the bodies of nerve-cells. The peripheral nerve-fibres are a continuation of the structures in the central nervous system, or arc composed of similar elements; the active part of the fibre, the axis-cylinder, is an outgrowth of the cytoplasm of the body of a nerve-cell, and the surrounding medullary sheath is a continuation of the material which sheathes the axis-cylinder while in the brain and cord. It is probable, therefore, that the chemistry of the axis-cylinder approaches to that of the cytoplasm of the body of the nerve- cell of which it is a branch, and that the chemistry of the medullary substance is the same outside as inside the central nervous system. The white matter of the brain of the ox, which is largely made up of nerve- fibres, is composed of about 70 parts water and 30 parts solids, about one-half the latter being cholesterin, about a quarter proteids and connective-tissue Bub- stanee, and about a quarter complex fatty bodies, neiiro-keratin, salts, chiefly potassium salts and phosphates, and traces of xanthin, hypoxanthin, etc Analysis of human sciatic nerve gives the following percentage for the principal organic constituent-: Proteids, 36.8; lecithin. 32.57; cholesterin and fat, 12.22; cerebrins, 11.30; neurokeratin, .'1.07 ; other substances, 1.0.- The nerve-fibre has a delicate sheath, the neurilemma, t he exact constitution 1 Zuntz: Pfluger'a Arehiv, L897, Bd. Ixx. 8. 346. 'J. chevalier: Zeitachrift fur phy&iologische Chemie, x. S. 97. 170 AN AMERICA X TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. of which is unknown, but which is supposed to resemble the sarcolemma and to be composed of a substance similar to elastin. The fibres are bound together by connective tissue which on boiling gives gelatin. Within the neurilemma is the medullary sheath, which is composed of two elements — viz. (1) neurokera- tin, a material similar to the horny substance of epithelial structures, which forms a sort of loose trellis, or network, and probably acts as a supporting framework to the fibre; (2) a white, highly refracting, semi-fluid material, which fills the meshes of the neuro-keratin network, and which is composed largely of protagon and cholesterin combined with fatty bodies. Protagon is a complex phosphorized nitrogenous compound, which many observers believe to contain lecithin and cerebrin. According to Noll, it makes up 7.47 per cent, of the dried nerve. Both lecithin and cerebrin are fatty bodies possess- ing nitrogen, and the former phosphorus. These and some other complex fatty bodies probably exist in addition to protagon in the medullary sub- tance. The formation of the "myelin forms" seen in the medulla of dead nerves is attributed to lecithin. The axis-cylinder probably contains most of the proteids of the fibre, chiefly globulins, mixed with complex fatty bodies. The reaction of the normal living fibre is neutral or slightly alkaline. It is said to become acid after death, but this change is not known to accompany functional activity. Indeed, nothing is known of the physiological import of the chemical constituents of the nerve-fibre or of the chemical changes which occur in the axis-cylinder when it develops or transmits the nerve impulse. The peculiar chemical composition of the medullary substance would suggest that it has a more important function than simply to protect the axis-cylinder. Some have attributed to it nutritive powers, and others have supposed it helped to insulate: it is certain that the axis-cylinder can develop and transmit the nerve impulse without the aid of the medullary sheath, for there is a large class of important nerves — the non-medullated nerves — in which it is lacking. II. CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. Introduction. The Unity of the Central Nervous System. — The human nervous system is formed by a mass of separate but contiguous nerve-cells. Indeed, a group of nerve-cells disconnected from the other nerve-tissues of the body, as the muscles or glands are disconnected from each other, would be without physiological significance. To understand, therefore, the physiology of the nervous system, it is important to keep in mind the fact that by dissection it is found to be continuous throughout its entire extent. When the nervous system is described as formed of a central and a peripheral portion, and the peripheral portion is further divided into its spinal and sympathetic components, the parts distinguished are found to have no sharply marked boundaries separating them, but really to merge one into the other. For topographical descriptions the convenience of such subdivisions is undoubted ; but the physiological processes which it is our purpose to study overstep in so large a measure these limits that the picture of events in the central nervous system would be very incomplete, should they be separately traced only within such prescribed anatomical boundaries. By virtue of its continuity the nervous system puts into connection all the other systems of the body. Conforming as it docs in shape to the frame- work of the body, its branches extend to all parts. These branches form pathwavs over which nerve-impulses travel toward the central system — the brain and spinal cord — and, in consequence of the impulses that come in, there pass out from the central system other impulses to the muscles and glands. All incoming impulses must reach the central system. It is a fad of the greatesl significance that until they have entered the central system the incoming impulses do not give rise to those outgoing, for thus all incoming impulses are first brought to the spinal cord and brain, where the outgoing impulses are co-ordinated. By means of the central system reactions are established in parts of the body not directly affected by the variation of the external conditions. < >wing also to the wide connections of the nervous system and the conduction of all incoming impulses to its central parts, a measure of harmony is maintained 171 172 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. between the activities of the several systems composing the body. Thus, not only the system.- forming the body are in this manner controlled, but the body as a whole, in relation to all things outside of it and forming; its environment, is even more plainly under the guidance of these administra- tive cells. Phenomena Involving- Consciousness. — It is with the brain and espe- cially the cerebral hemispheres that the phenomena of consciousness are most closely linked. Strictly, physiology concerns itself, at present, with the reac- tions of the aervous system which can he studied without an appeal to con- sciousness. A moment's consideration shows, however, that in the physiology of the brain the assistance to be obtained by passing beyond the limit thus laid down is of more value than any boundary, and hence, although the field of consciousness is sacred to psychology, physiology should not be deprived of any of the advantages which come from the privilege of occasional trespass. Growth and Organization. — The physiological connections existing be- tween the nerve-elements in infancy are very incomplete and poorly estab- lished, more so than in any other system of the body ; in the history of the growth of the nervous system, the increase in weight and change in shape run parallel with an increase in its complexity. This increase in complexity i- accompanied by more perfect organization, which results in better and more numerous physiological pathways, thus permitting the system, as a whole, not only to do more perfectly at maturity those things which it could do in some degree at an earlier age, but also, by virtue of its increased com- plexity, to do at maturity those things which previously it could not do at all. Growth in the case of this system implies, therefore, an increase in com- plexity such as nowhere else occurs, and, since this growth can be modified by the experience of the individual during the growing period, the impor- tance of understanding it and its relation to organization is evident. Plan of Presentation. — Our subject may be discussed under three heads dealing respectively with the physiology of the (1) single cells, (2) groups of • •ells, ami (•"> i the entire central system. Part I. The physiology of the nerve-cells, considered as a peculiar kind of tissue-element, endowed with special physiological characters. Part II. The activities of groups of these elements. The physiological grouping is, of course, mainly dependent on the anatomical arrangement. At the same time, the activities of one group modify those of others. Stated in general terms, the problem in this pari is that of the pathway of any imj)ii/.<, through tin- central system. Part III. The reactions of the system taken as a whole. Here its capa- bilities as a unit are contrasted with those of the other systems, and its growth, organization, and rhythms of resl and activity are most readily de- scribed. CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. 173 PART I.— PHYSIOLOGY OF THE NERVE-CELL. A. Anatomical Characteristics of the Nerve-cell. Form of Nerve-cells. — Morphologically the mature nerve-eel 1 is regarded as composed of a cell-body, containing a nucleus, together with other modified inclusions, and possessed of one or more outgrowths or branches. Some of these branches may be very long, such, for instance, as those which form nerve-fibres ; other branches are short and differ from the nerve-fibres in their structure. The terms employed in describing the nerve-elements are as follows : To the entire mass under the control of a given nucleus and forming both cell- body and branches, the term neurone is applied. The inclusions within the ' , / ^> o Fig. 67. — A group of human nerve-cells, all drawn to the same scale, from pioparations according to Nissl's method, made by Dr. Adolph Meyer, and kindly furnished for this purpose; X 300: a, small motor cell from ventral horn of cervical spinal cord ; b, cell from " Clarke's column," thoracic cord ; c, small nerve-cell from tip of dorsal horn, thoracic cord ; d, spinal ganglion-cell, cervical root ; > . three granules from the granular layer of the cerebellum; /, l'urkinje's cell from the same preparation as « ; be two nerve-fibres arising from the cell-body, in others, only one. For conve- nience the descriptions about to be given will apply to the latter group only. From most cells there arises one principal branch, which when considered alone is described as a nerve-fibre, but when considered as the outgrowth of the cell-body from which it originates is called the axone. The axone, in many cases, has branches, both near its origin from the cell-body and also along its course. These branches are designated as collaterals. At their distal ends the main stem of the axone and also the collaterals subdivide 174 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. into finer twigs, forming the terminal arborizations. Contrasted with this principal outgrowth are the other branches of the cell, which are individu- ally much less extensive and which divide dichotomously at frequent in- tervals. From the tree-like form which they thus acquire they have been designated :?;~ ?f rr-^. -? ~~^~7 '/** of careful consideration. First, there is good indirect evidence that, in early life at least, and before their branches have been formed, they are migratory, moving in an amoeboid manner. This being so, the perfection with which they arrange themselves in the adult system depends on the accuracy with which they respond to those conditions that deter- mine their migration as well as upon the normal character of these directing influ- ences (mechanical strain;1 chemotaxis ; nu- tritive attraction or electrical influences).3 But with so much liberty of movement and with directing influences that are so compli- cated, the chances for deviation from a fixed arrangement are much enhanced. Second, very early in the history of the neuroblast the point on the cell-body from which the axone will grow appears in many x lioodiametera (His) .a, germinal ceil; cases to fa determined, and the cell is thus A', neuroblasts. physiologically polarized.3 This polarity being established, the direction in which the axone first grows is determined, and where the cells are misplaced this polarization can lead to a confusion of arrangement. The volume of either the germinal cell or of the first form of the neuro- blast was found by His' to be 697 cubic fi in a human fetus (embryo It- length 5.5 millimeters), aged 3-3.5 weeks ; and it can be shown that the mature neurone must often attain a volume more than 50,000 times that of the orig- inal neuroblast. 1 His: Unsere Kin- perform, 1874. 2 Davenport: Bulletin of the Museum of Comparative Zoology, Harvard College, Nov., 1895; Hcrl.st: Biologiaekea Centralblatt, 1894, Bd., xiv.; H. Strasser: Ergebnme der Anatamie u. Ent- wickclunr/.tr/esrfiirlite, Merkel and Bonnet, 1891, Bd. i. S. 731. 3 Mall: Journal of Morphology, 1893, vol. viii. ' Arrhir fur Anatomie mid Pkysiologie, 1889. Fig. 69.— Portion of developing medul lary tube (human) seen in frontal section CENTRA L NER VO US S ) 'S TKM. 177 Maturing- of the Nerve-cell. — The maturing of the nerve-cell involves several changes. First, the outgrowth of the axone or axones; next, the formation of the dendrites ; and, finally, in some cases, the medullatiou of the axone, while simultaneously and with greater or less rapidity the absolute amount of substance in both cell-body and axone is being increased, together with a chemical differentiation of the cytoplasm and the nucleus. The time in the life-history of the individual at which these several events occur is variable, and may be delayed beyond puberty at least, while the rate at which they occur is diiferent in different cases. Furthermore, many nerve-cells never develop beyond the first stage of immaturity (Fig. 70). Fig. 70.— A-D, showing the phylogenetic development "f mature nerve-cella in a series of vertebrates ; a-e, the ontogenetic development of growing cells in a typical mammal; in both ra>.-v only pyramidal cells from the cerebrum are shown : .1, frog ; /•', lizard ; ''. rut ; />, man ; o, neuroblast withoul dendrites ; 6, commencing dendrites ; r, dendrites further developed ; , further development of collaterals ami dendrites (from S. Ram6n y Cajal). Form of the Axone as a Means of Classification. — Of the various de- vices used to classify nerve-cells, the form of the axone is the most useful. Physiologically, the nerve-cell is significant as the source and pathway for the nerve-impulses. The current conception of the change called the nerve- impulse is that it begins at one point of the cell and travels from there to the other parts ; one of the other parts is the axone. and along this the impulse can be shown to pass. Indeed, the nerve-cell body stimulated at any point may be responsive just an an amoeba is responsive at any portion of its sur- face. When, however, the branches are formed they become the channel- through which the impulses pass, and hence assume a special significance Voi,. i r. — 12 178 AN AMERICAN TEXT- BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. without indicating any fundamental change in the structure of the cell. Where the cell lias well-developed branches we explain the arrangement by assuming thai tli<' impulse enters the cell-body by one branch and leaves it by another. On examining the mature nerve-cells of man with this idea in mind, two types are found. The first type maybe exemplified by the pyramidal corti- cal cells shown in fig. 70. Here, from a pyramidal body (D) there arise a number of dendrites, while from the lower portion of the cell the axone Fig. 71.— Spinal ganglion of an embryo duck; composed of diaxonic nerve-cells (van Gehuchten). grows out and branches. In the other type the axone alone grows out. Its branches are but two in number and both are medullated. They pass in oppo- site directions, and in this type there are, as a rule, no dendrites. Such are the typical spinal ganglion-cells of the mammal. To understand the arrangement in these cases, recourse must be had to the facts of develop- ment. The second type begins its development as a diaxonic cell, an axone growing from each pole (Fig. 71). In the adult spinal ganglion of the higher mammals, however, such diaxonic cells are rarely found, the great majority having a single axone which soon divides into two branches.1 Fig. 72.— Diaxonic changing into monaxonic cells : from the Gasserian ganglion of a developing guinea-pig (van Gehuchten). Fig. 1'2 beautifully illustrates the phases of this change as seen in a sin- gle section. At first one axone arises from each pole of the ovoid cell-body. Later the cell-body occupies a position at the side of the two axones, which appear to run into one other. Finally the cell-body is separated from the two axones by an intervening stem. The stem has the characters of a medul- lated nerve-fibre, and from the end of it the two original axones pass off as branches. 1 Dogiel: Anal. Am. Jeva, 1890, Bel. xii. S. 140-152, describes the several kinds of neu- rones which take part in the formation of the spinal ganglion. CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. 179 From this mode of development it is plain that the single stem must be looked upon as containing a double pathway, although it appears to be in all ways a single fibre, for on the one hand it contains the path for the incoming and on the other for the outgoing impulses. Recent investigations have shown in a striking way that cells modified in this manner are by no means limited to the spinal ganglia, but occur in the cortex of the cerebellum and elsewhere. Classifying the nerve-cells, therefore, in the light of these facts, we find : (1) The pyramidal type, in which the dendrites and axone are both well developed, and in which the greater number of the impulses most probably enter the cell by way of the dendrites and leave by way of the axone ; (2) The spinal ganglion type, in which originally the impulse passes in at one pole of the cell and out at the other, but in the course of development the twoaxones become attached to the cell-body by a single stem, and by inference there must be in this stem a double pathway. In this latter case there are usually no dendrites. Growth of Branches. — After the cells have taken on their type-form the branches still continue to grow, not only in length, but also in diameter. In man, for example, the diameter of the nerve-fibres (axones) taken from the peripheral nerves at birth is 1.2-2 fi for the smallest, up to 7-8 // for the largest, with an average of 3-4 /x, while at maturity it is 10-15 [i for the larger fibres.1 Internal Structure of the Neurones. — The status of this problem has been admirably summarized by Barker,2 to whose book the reader is referred. For our purpose it is sufficient to state that the cytoplasm of nerve-cells is composed of fibrils (the character of which is much discussed), and an inter- mediate, non-fibrillar material. These constituents are distributed in different proportions in the several parts of the neurone. The axone contains the fibrils most closely packed. The intermediate substance is most evident in the body of the cell, and in general the dendrites more closely resemble in their structure the cell body. Part, at least, of the intermediate material forms the " stainable substance" of Nissl, also called "tigroid," which, in its susceptibility to change under disturbed nutritive conditions, acts like a stored food material. But which portion of the cell acts to conduct the nerve im- pulse is not known, and the contention that one or the other of* the compo- nent structures is the conductor of the nerve impulses rests on histological evidence alone. For the present it is sufficient to know that the neurone appears to be conductive in all its gross parts. While the axone is growing as a naked axis-cylinder, it is usually slivjitly enlarged at the tip (Cajal), suggesting thai it is specially modified at that point. The nutritive exchange on which the increase of the entire axone depends appears to take place along its whole extent, and no! l" be entirely dependent on material passed from the cell-body into the axone. Medullation. — After the production of its several branches, the next step 1 Westphal : Neurologisches Centralblalt, 1894, No. 2. 'Barker: The Nervous System, L 899, pp. 101-114. 180 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. in the growth of the cell is the formation of the medullary sheath about the axone. Not all axones have a medullary sheath, nor is any axone com- pletely medullated. In the sympathetic system there is a very large pro- portion of unmedullated axones. In the central system the number is also very large, although their mass is small. Of the significance of the medullary sheath we know nothing-. The suggestion that it acts to insulate the nerve impulse within a giveD axis-cylinder has little or no evidence in its favor. The suggestion that it is nutritive is plausible, but important differences in the physiological reactions of the two classes of nerve-fibres have not vet been found, if we except the observation that the non -medullated nerves rapidly lose their irritability at the point of stimulation with the faradic current, thus exhibiting a "stimulation fatigue" not found in nerves unquestionably medullated. Growth of Medullary Sheath in Peripheral Nerves. — Whatever may be the significance of the medullary sheath, it is usually formed before the neurone has attained its full size. In the peripheral system it depends on the presence of mesodermal cells which envelop the axis-cylinder, forming a tube about it. Each ensheathing cell is physiologically controlled by a nu- Fig. 73.— Longitudinal (B\ and transverse (A) sections of nerve-fibres. The heavy border represents the medullary sheath, which is thicker in the larger fibres. Human sciatic nerve. X200 diameters (modified from van Gehuchten). cleus which becomes situated about midway between its extremities. Accord- ing to Ranvier and his school, the cell-substance is largely transformed into myelin, and the line of junction between two of these sheathing cells forms a node of the nerve-fibre. In the sheath of a growingaxone at least two changes can be readily followed : As the axis-cylinder increases in diameter the medul- lary sheath becomes thicker. The change is such that in the mammalian peripheral system the areas of the axis-cylinder and of the medullary sheath, as shown in cross-sections of osmic acid preparations, remain nearly equal (Fig. 73). On the other hand, the length of the intemodal segments tends to increase with an increase in the diameter of the nerve-fibre, and for nerves of the same diameter it is less in man than in the lower animals. In a given fibre the segments are shorter at the extreme peripheral end ( Key and Retz- ius). In the young fibres, also, they are shorter and increase in length with age. A physiological significance attaches to these segments, because, as Ran- vier long since pointed out. it is at the nodes that various staining reagents easily reach the axis-cylinder. This suggests that normal nutritive exchanges may follow the same path, and thus short intemodal segments giving rise to CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. 181 many nodes would represent the condition most favorable to exchange be- tween the axis-cylinder and the surrounding plasma. Thus far, histological observation shows the more numerous nodes where the physiological processes are presumptively most active, and hence supports the hypothesis suggested. In the peripheral nervous system the nerve-fibres conduct impulses before they acquire their medullary sheaths : witness the activities of new-born rats, in which the whole nervous system is entirely unmedullated. Moreover, Langley x has reported, in the regenerating cervical sympathetic nerve, a return of function, while the majority of the fibres are still without their medullary sheaths. Medullation in Central System. — Concerning the relation of the medul- lary sheath to the axis-cylinder in the central system, our information is less complete. The elements which give rise to the medullary substance are not known and the myelin is not enclosed in a primitive sheath. There are no internodal nuclei regularly placed, yet Porter2 has demonstrated in both the frog and the rabbit the existence of nodes in some fibres taken from the spinal cord. The conditions which there exist must be further studied before any general statements concerning the development of the medullary substance in the nerve-centres can be ventured. Yet, it is an important observation that whereas medullation in the peripheral system is mainly, completed during the first five years of life, the process continues in the central system, and espe- cially in the cerebral cortex, to beyond the fiftieth year.3 Whatever views may be held concerning the capacities of a medullated fibre, it is to be remembered that the medullary sheath does not cover the first part of the axone on its emergence from the cell-body, nor are ultimate branches of the axone medullated in the region of their final distribution. What has just been said applies to the main stem of the axone. As shown in Fig. 70, the axone often has branches near its origin, the collaterals, and according to the observations of Flechsig4 these also become medullated. Concerning the time of the medullation of these branches there are no direct observations; but if it is controlled by the same conditions which appear to control the process in the main stem, then, as the branches form their physio- logical connections later than the main stem, it would follow thai their medullation should also occur later, and the studies on the progressive medul- lation of the cerebral cortex favor such a view. The acquisition of this sheath occurs in response to a physiological change that appears at the same time along the entire length of tin1 fibre. Tin1 proc- ess, therefore, is not a progressive one, but is practically simultaneous. From the observations of Ambronn and Held'' on rabbits a day or two old, it appears that the efferent (motor) spinal ami cranial nerves acquire their 1 Langley : Journal of Physiology, 1897, xxii. p. 223. - Quarterly Journal of Microscopical Science, 1890. 3Vulpius: Archiv fiir Psychiatric u/nd Nervenkrank., 1S92, lid. xxiii. 4 Archiv fur Anatomic unci Physiologic, 1889. •''Ambronn and Held : Archiv fiir Anatomic and Physiologic, Anatom. A.btb.1., 1896, S. 208. 182 AN AMERICAN TEXT- BOOK OE PHYSIOLOGY. sheaths before the corresponding afferent (sensory) nerves are medullated (except in the ease of the vestibular branch of the auditory nerve, which is medullated at the same time as the motor nerves). In the central system the continuations of the afferenl pathways become medullated before the pyramidal tracts, while in the cerebral hemispheres medullation of the com- missural and association fibres follows immediately that of the afferent tracts. Ambrmin and Held1 have also shown that when the eyelids are prema- turely opened in animals born blind, such as the rabbit, dog or cat, and the animal is then exposed to the light, the medullary sheaths are more rapidly formed in the optic nerves exposed to stimulation than in those developing normally. Changes in the Cytoplasm. — While the nerve-cell is passing from the immature to the mature form, increasing in mass and in the number of its branches, as well as acquiring its medullary sheath, it also undergoes various chemical changes. The stainable substance in the cytoplasm becomes more abundant at maturity and the pigment-granules increase in quantity.2 Fig. 74.— To show the changes in nerve-cells due to age : A, spinal ganglion-cells of a still-born male child : B. spinal ganglion-cells of a man dying at ninety-two years ; n, nuclei. In the old man the cells are not large, the cytoplasm is pigmented, the nucleus is small, and the nucleolus much shrunken or absent. Both sections taken from the first cervical ganglion, X 250 diameters (Hodge). Old Age of the Nerve-cells. — But the nerve-cell, though possessing in most cases a life-history coextensive with that of the entire body, eventually exhibits retrogressive changes. These changes of old age consist, in some measure, in a reversal of those processes most evident during active growth. The cell-body, together with the nucleus and its subdivisions, becomes smaller, the stainable substance diminishes and becomes diffused instead of appearing in compact masses,' the pigment increases, the cytoplasm exhibits vacuoles, the dendrites atrophy, and the axones also probably diminish in mass. In some instances the entire cell is absorbed. Some of these factsare illustrated by the observations of Hodge4 on the spinal ganglion-cells of an old man of ninety-two year- ;is compared with those of a new-born child (see Fig. 74). Since the chemical and morphological variations which occur during the entire growth-cycle are accompanied by variations in the physiological powers, 1 hoc. cit., S. 222. * Vas: Archiv fur mikroskopische Anatomie, 1892. 3 Marinesco : Revue neurologique, October, 1899, No. 20. 1 Journal of Physiology, 1894, vol. xvii. CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. 183 we are led to anticipate in old age a correlation, on the one hand, between the decrease in the quantity of functional substance in the cytoplasm, and a decrease in the energy-producing power of the cells, and, on the other, between the absorption of the cell-branches and a Limitation in the extenf to which the neurones may influence one another. Both of these conditions are characteristic of the nervous system during old age. B. The Nerve-impulse within a Single Neurone. The Nerve-impulse. — Neurones form the pathways along which nerve- impulses travel. As introductory, therefore, to the study of the composite pathways in the central system, comprising, as they do, several elements ar- ranged in series, it becomes important to study the behavior of the nerve- impulse within the limits of a single cell-element. Experimentally, the passage of the nerve-impulse is revealed by a wave of change in the form of an electrical variation which passes along the nerve- fibre in both directions from the point of stimulation. Under normal condi- tions, the intensity of the electrical change does not vary in transit, though for moderate electrical stimuli the strength of the electrical change ("action current") is proportional to the strength of the stimulus.1 It moves in the peripheral nerves of the frog in the form of a wave some 18 millimeters in length, at the mean rate of 30 meters per second, and this rate can be some- what retarded by cooling the nerves and accelerated by warming them. In mammals the rate in the peripheral nerves has been found by Helmholtz and Baxt to be 34 meters per second, The nerve-impulse can be aroused at any point on a nerve-fibre*provided a sufficient length of fibre be subjected to stimulation. Mechanical, thermal, chemical, and electrical stimuli may be used to arouse it, but just how the impulse thus started differs from that normally passing along the fibres, as a consequence of changes in the cell- bodies of which these fibres are outgrowths, is not known. It appears, how- ever, that the impulses aroused by artificial stimuli are usually accompanied by a much stronger electrical variation than accompanies the normal impulses. In the peripheral system the nerve-impulse, when once started within a fibre or axone, is confined to that track and does not diffuse toother fibres running parallel with it, although it does, of course, extend to all the branches of the axone, whatever their distribution. The above-mentioned relations have been deduced from the study of die peripheral nerves, and these morphologically are but parts of the axones, the cell-bodies of which are located either in the central system proper or in the spinal or sympathetic ganglia. The observations apply therefore to but one portion of the nerve-cell, and our present purpose is to determine how far it is possible to apply them to the entire nerve-cell, noting at the same time the modifications thus intro- duced. Owing to the small size of nerve-cell bodies there arc. of course, very lew '(Ireene: American Journal of Plti/siotoi/i/, 1NJIS, vol. i. p. 1 15. 184 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. instances in which a single nerve-cell, or part of such a cell, has been the object of direct physiological experiment. We shall therefore approach the question indirectly by showing- what the histological relations have to suggest. Direction of the N6rve-irnpulse. — In the case of a given nerve-cell the impulses which we usually consider, pa>> in one direction only. For instance, along the ventral nerve-roots of the spinal cord, the impulses pass from the cord to the periphery, while in the dorsal roots, so far as the fibres take origin from the cells of the spinal ganglia, these impulses travel in the oppo- site direction. At the same time experiment has shown that if a nerve- trunk be stimulated at a given point, then the nerve-impulse can be demon- strated as passing away from the point of stimulation in both directions. We are therefore led to inquire what limits are set to the passage of impulses in a direction opposite to the usual one. The narrowest limits, it appears, are those of the single cell in which the impulse has originated. The experimental observations are as follows : When the fibres forming the ventral root of a spinal nerve are stimulated electrically, and the cross- section of the spinal cord, somewhat cephalad to the level at which the root joins it, is explored with an electrometer, there is not found any evidence of nerve-impulses passing cephalad in the substance of the cord. The arrange- ment of the cells in the cord is such, however, that the cell-bodies which give origin to the fibres forming the ventral root are physiologically controlled by fibres running toward them from every portion of the cord, and under normal conditions these fibres convey impulses to the cell-bodies in question. The experiment shows that when an impulse enters the cell-body by way of the ventral root-fibre, to which it gives origin, the impulse* does not stimulate the other elements of the cord.1 With the elements forming the dorsal spinal root the case is at first glance apparently different, though in reality it is the same. These elements have the cell-body located in the spinal ganglion. The cells are essentially diaxonic (Fig. 72); one axone extends from the point of division toward the periphery and the other enters the spinal cord, where it forms two branches, both of which course longitudinally for some distance within it (see Fig. 75). In this case, therefore, the normal direction of the effective impulses is from the periphery toward the cord, and within the cord they are delivered to other elements, which carry them in all directions. It is there- fore to be expected that the stimulation of the dorsal root-fibres would give rise to impulses passing in both directions in the dorsal columns of the cord. When, however, the dorsal columns of the cord are electrically stimulated in a cross-section made just above the level of the entrance of a dorsal root, then it is found that the electrical variation is to be detected in the nerve- fibers on the distal side of the spinal ganglion. These impulses have there- fore passed in a direction the reverse of that usually taken. The fibres which in this instance are stimulated in the cross-section of the cord are, however, outgrowths of tin; spinal ganglion-cells, and thus, although the 1 Gotch and Ilorsley : Proceedings of the Royal Society, 1888. CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. 185 stimulation of the cord does give rise to an impulse in the afferent spinal nerve, nevertheless the impulse is continually within the limits of one cell- element. This shows that the reversed impulse can pass the spinal ganglion, and in doing this it probably traverses the cell-bodies there located. There is, however, no evidence that the stimulation of the dorsal columns of the cord produces out- going impulses in the dorsal nerve-roots except when the stimulus is applied to the axones, which are outgrowths of the cells of the spinal ganglia. In the case of the interpolation of the cell- body in the course of the axones there is every reason to think that the nerve-impulse traverses the body of the cell itself. This is suggested by the changes caused in the cell-body of the spinal ganglion-cells as the result of stimulating the peripheral axone. Moreover, some observers report an appreciable delay (0.036 second) in the passage of the nerve-impulse through the cell-body in the case of those cells which form the spinal ganglion.1 This delay has recently been denied.2 The observations of Steinach,3 on the capacity of the afferent nerves of the frog to conduct the centripetal impulses through the region of the spinal ganglion, indicate that impulses may pass this region when the cell-bodies are very prob- ably excluded from forming a part of the possi- ble pathways, thus showing that tho two branches of the T-process are physiologically continuous. These results do not show, however, that the centripetal impulses fail, under normal condi- tions, to pass to the cell-bodies also. It may be pointed out that this is another piece of evi- dence in favor of the view that within the limits of a single neurone or fraction of a neurone there is no limitation to the passage of a nerve-impulse in all directions, wherever it is started. Double Pathways. — [f the view is correct, that in passing through the spinal ganglion the normal impulse traverses the cell-body, then the nerve- Pro. 75.— A longitudinal section of the cord to show the branshing of incoming root-fihres in dorsal columns. At the left are three (DR) root-fihres, each of which forms two principal branches. These give off at right angles other branches, col- laterals, 0>l, which terminate in brushes. O C, central cells, whose axones give off similar collaterals (Ramon y Cajal). 'Gad and Joseph : Archivf. Anatomie ». Physiologic, 1889. 2 Moore and Reynolds: Proceedings of the Fourth International Physiological Congress, held at Cambridge, 1898. Supplement, vol. xxiii, Journal of Physiology. These authors deny the delay. 3 Steinach: " Ueber die centripetale Erregungsleitnng im Iterciche des Spinalganglions," Pfiiiger's Archiv, 1899, Bd. 78. 186 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OE PHYSIOLOGY. impulse passes to and fro along the common stem which joins the cell-body with the two branches [vide Fig. 72), the stem itself having all the characters of a medullated iibre. The study of this modification brings with it the following suggestion : It' the single stem in the modified spinal ganglion-cells must by virtue of its development contain a double pathway, it is fair to inquire whether the same may not be true of the other forms of the nerve-cell in which the axone also appears to be single. Among the cortical cells the arrangement of the branches is such that, for aught that is known, the stem of the axone may functionate in the manner suggested, and contain more than one pathway. The same arrangement must exist in the case of cells like those repre- sented in Fig. 76, in which the axone arises from the base of a dendrite Fig. 76.— Showing the relations between the terminal branches of the dendrites (D) and of the axones (V) of the optic fibres where they come together in the superficial layer of the optic lobe of the chick ; also showing the origin of the axone (N) from a dendrite (van Gehuchten). at some distance from the cell-body, and in which nerve-impulses arriving over the dendrites and leaving by the axone must normally follow the por- tion of the cell-branch which is common to both, passing along it first in one direction and then in the other. This last result has been extended by Sherrington,1 who found that he could produce movements of the hind limb in both monkeys and cats when the cord had been sectioned just below the bulb, and the stimulus was applied to the fibres in the fasciculi graciles at that level. The reaction is explained by the passage of impulses down the dorsal columns (in a direction reverse to the normal), and their distribution by way of the collaterals to the efferent elements located in the ventral horns. Significance of Cell-Branches. — Since the outgoing nerve-impulses are isolated in the axone until they reach the terminal twigs, it follows that the impulses destined to produce an effect beyond the cell limits will do so at the extremities of the branches. This leads to the question how far the posses- sion of branches is necessary to the functional activity of a nerve-cell either for the reception or transmission of an impulse. Since it has been pointed out that the spinal cord of the newt and fish is capable of conducting impulses even before the dendrites are developed, it follows that the transmission of impulses is in some way dependent on the condition of the cell-wall, inde- pendent of cell-branches. This modification may exist at points where there are no branches, or during this early period be a general property of the 1 Proceedings of the Iioyal Society, 1897, lxi. 243-246. CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. 187 wall, and only later become the peculiar property of those portions which are drawn out to form the tips of the branches. But not only the capacity to receive, but also the capacity to deliver impulses is a function of the ends of the branches, and the cell-wall at these points must therefore be peculiarly modified with a still further differentiation, determining the direction in which the impulses may pass. Each dendrite may represent at least one pathway by which impulses reach the cell-body. If, then, there are many dendrites, the cell-body is subject to a more complicated series of stimuli than if the branches are few. It will be remembered that the young nerve-cell has no dendrites, that the first branch to be formed is the axone, and that the com- pletion of the full number of dendrites is a slow process. The pathways formed by the dendrites are therefore continually increasing up to maturity Fig. 77.— Climbing fibre from human brain : a, nerve-fibre ; b, Purkinje's cell (Cajal). (Fig. 77). The relation between the " climbing fibre " and the dendrites of the Purkinje cell illustrates this arrangement. Generation of Nerve-impulses. — The impulses which arrive at the cell- body produce there chemical changes. These changes, when they reach a given volume, cause a nerve-impulse which leaves the cell-body by way of the axone. If the nerve-impulse is, as we assume, dependent on the chem- ical changes occurring in the cytoplasm, then it must vary according to these changes, which in turn can hardly be similar when the incoming impulses that arouse them arrive along different dendrites. We know thai a stimulus applied directly to the axone will give rise to a nerve-impulse; but, as we shall see later, the chemical changes accompanying the passage of this impulse are too slight to be detected. Whether in the cell-body equally slight changes would give rise to an impulse cannot be determined. Birge1 found upon stabbing the spinal cord of a frog with a needle in 1 Birge: Arch./. Anat. u. Physiol., Physiol. Abthl., 1882, S. 471. 188 AN AMERICAN TEXT- BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. the region where ftie efferent cell-bodies are clustered together, that not only were impulses sent out of the cord, causing the muscles to contract, but they continued to be sent out for some seconds after the injury. When an electrical stimulus is applied to the cerebral cortex so as to stimulate the cells there present, the discharging cells may also continue to send out impulses for some time after the cessation of the stimulus. Experiments showing the multiple character of the impulses aroused within the central system have been made by Gotch and Horsley.1 When the motor cortex of a monkey was stimulated (Fig. 78) by means of the faradic current, the muscles which by this means were made to respond showed a long tonic contraction followed by a series of shorter clonic ones (Fig. 79, D). When the spinal cord had been cut across, the cortex was again stimulated, and the electrical changes in the fibres of the cord which convey the impulses from the cortex to the spinal centres were investigated by means of the capillary electrometer. By this means a curve (Fig. 79, D) was obtained as a record of the negative variations passing along these fibres. This latter curve corresponds with the record for the muscular contraction, and hence the relation between the two series of events is evident. It appears, therefore, that the cortical cells after the cessa- -Mercury. ^Sulphuric acid 10%. Microscope. nj Miriiiri/. tion of the stimulus still continue to dis- charge in a rhyth- mical manner. When the cortex had been removed, and the electrodes were ap- plied directly to the underlying fibres, the discharge of the im- pulses was found to cease with the stoppage of the stimulus. The presence of the cortex was therefore necessary for the continued discharge (Fig. 79, C). The attempt 1 Proceedings of the Royal Society, London, 1888. Fig. 78.— Schema illustrating the experiment fur determining the num- ber of separate nerve-impulses passing down the spinal cord upon stimula- tion of the cortex (from experiments on the monkey ; Horsley) : E, E. elec- trodes, intended to be on the "leg area." Where the cord is interrupted, one non-polarizable electrode is placed over the cut end of the pyramidal fibres going to the lumbar enlargement ; the other, on the side of the cord. These lead to the capillary electrometer, in which the column of mercury moves each time an impulse passes. CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. 189 was also made to determine the rhythmic character of the negative varia- tions in the motor nerve-trunk between the cord and the contracting muscle, but the changes there present, though sufficient to cause contractions of the muscle, were not strong enough to be recorded by a delicate capillary electrom- eter. This result suggests that the impulses sent out from the spinal cord by the normal discharge of the motor nerve-cells may differ from the impulses artificially aroused in the lesser intensity of the electrical changes that accompany them. Rate of Discharge. — The rate at which the nerve-cells discharge, as shown by the number of impulses which produce tetanus of a muscle indi- rectly excited, either by artificial stimulation of the nerve-elements in animals or by voluntary impulses in man, is about ten impulses per second. It appears that at least the cortical cells and those of the spinal cord have the same rate of discharge, and that this rate is the same in sonic mammals (dogs, cats, rabbits, and monkeys) as in man. Hence a tendency to discharge about ten times a second may be assumed as characteristic of the mammalian nerve-cell.1 Points at which the Nerve-impulse can be Aroused. — It is probable that the excitation of any part of a nerve-cell is capable of producing a nerve- D < w*. I Excitation. | I 1 Sec. | Excitation. I I i I I I I I I I Sec- I Fig. 79. — From a photographic record of the movements of the column of mercury in a capillary electrometer (Uotch and Horsley). The arrow shows the direction in which the record is to be read The upper curve {D) shows the period of excitation by the interrupted current; this is followed by a series of waves in the record showing a number of separate impulses sent down from the cortex after electrical stimulation has ceased. In the lower curve (C), the exciting electrodes were applied to the white matter directly, the cortex having been removed. The record shows that in this case no impulses pass after the stimulation has ceased. impulse, whether the stimulus be applied at the tips of the dendrites or t<> the axone in its course. Irritability and Conductivity. — In general, parts of the system which are irritable are also conductive, but there are special cases in which the irritability of the nerve-fibre can be distinctly separated from its conductivity, the latter being present while the former is absent. It is an old observation thai on stripping down the phrenic nerve by compressing it between the thumb and forefinger and sliding these along the nerve, a contraction of the diaphragm is caused. The part of the nerve thus stimulated is soon exhausted. If, now, the same operation is repeated on a portion of the nerve lying nearer the spinal cord, contraction of the diaphragm again follows. This result was originally used to support the theory of a 'Schliferand Horsley: Journal of Physiology, 1885, vol. vii.; Schafer, Ibid. 190 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. nerve-fluid, and was held to demonstrate that after the nerve-tubes in the portion ol* the trunk compressed had been emptied so that no reaction followed further pressure, then if the pressure were applied still nearer the cord, the fluid from that part of the nerve could be driven forward and a contraction of the diaphragm would result. The notion of a nerve-fluid in the sense in which that term was used by the earlier physiologists has long since been abandoned; but for our purpose, the experiment is important as showing that under such treatment irritability and conductivity do not dis- appear at the same time, but that the fibres remain conductive after they cease to be irritable. It has been shown also that1 in young regenerating motor-fibres it often happens that while no response is to be obtained by the direct stimulation of the regenerated peripheral portion, yet the stimulation of the central and fully grown portion does cause a contraction of the muscles controlled by these fibres. In this case the newly formed fibres can conduct an impulse which gives rise to a contraction, although such an impulse cannot be aroused by directly stimulating them. Summation of Stimuli in Nerve-cells. — In an axone a single stimulus if followed by a single nerve-impulse ; on the other hand, the studies which have been made to determine the number of weak stimuli necessary to dis- charge afferent cell-elements, when stimulated by way of the afferent nerves, indicate that there may be a summation of stimuli — i. c, the discharge does not follow until a scries of stimuli has been given.2 Whether, however, the delay in the response is due to the failure of the cytoplasm of the receiving cell to discharge until repeated impulses have reached it, or whether the modification of the cell which causes the delay is a process taking place at the point where the impulse passes over from the branches of one cell to those of another, is not directly determined by the experiments. The indirect evidence is, however, entirely in favor of the view that the delay which is notable in the arousal of a reflex response occurs at the point where the impulse passes from one cell to another. C. The Nutrition of the Nerve-cell. The metabolic processes within the nerve-cell are continuous, and the chemical changes there taking place involve not only those prerequisite to the enlargement of the cell during growth, but also those leading to the formation of such substances as by their katabolism cause the nerve-impulse. The passage of the nerve-impulses probably alters the osmotic powers of the cell-wall toward the surrounding plasma, and this of course is fundamental to the nutritive exchange. It follows, therefore, that the passage of nerve- impulses is one factor determining the nutrition of these cells. Histologically we look upon the cell-bodies as the part in which the most 1 Howell and Ilnber: Journal of Physiology, 1892, vol. xiii. 1 Ward: Archiv f. Anatomie u. Physiologie, 1880 ; Stirling: Arbeiten aus den physiologischen Anstalt in Leipzig, 1874. CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. 191 active changes occur, since the network of blood-vessels is most dense about them, indicating that the metabolic processes are here most active1 (Fig. 80). Chemical Changes. — For the direct microchemical determination of special substances within the nerve-cells there are but few methods, though some phosphorus-bearing substances (nucleins) can be demonstrated,2 and the occurrence of chemical changes due to activity and to age are very evident. Macallum3 has demonstrated the presence of iron in the stainable substance of Nissl. There is general consensus that the alkalinity of the nerve-tissues is decreased during activity, and this decrease in alkalinity may amount at times to a positively acid reaction. This change, too, is better supported by the observations made where the cell-bodies are numerous than by those made where the fibres are alone present. Fatigue. — Not only is the food-supply to the nerve-cells, as represented by the quality and quantity of the plasma, variable, but the cells themselves Fig. 80.— Frontal section through the human mid-brain at the level of the anterior quadrigeminum (Shimamura). On the left side the blood-vessels have been injected; on the right the gray matter is indicated by the heavy lines. It appears by this that the blood-vessels are most abundant in the gray matter. are subject to wide variations in their power to utilize these food materials, and deviations from the normal in either of these respects means a diminu- tion in the physiological powers of the cell, which we may call fatigue. In the nervous system the signs of fatigue are both physiological and histological, but it is to the latter changes only that attention will be here directed. If a faradic current is applied intermittently to the mixed nerve-trunk going to a limb, changes are to be observed in the cell-bodies belonging to the spinal ganglia of the several roots forming the nerve (Hodge). When this experiment is made on a cat, and, alter death, the sections from the stimulated are compared with those of the corresponding, but mi- stimulated, spinal ganglion, a picture like that represented by Fig. 81, is obtained.4 1 Shimamura: Neurologisches CerUralblalt, 1894, Bd. xiii. ! Lilienfeld and Monti : Zeitschrift fur physiologische Chemie, L892, Bd. xvii. 3 Macallum : British Medical Journal, London, 1898, vol. ii. p. 778. 4 Hodge : Journal of Morphology, 1S92. 192 AN AMERICAN TEXT- BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. The sections indicate that the cytoplasm, together with the enclosed nucleus and nucleolus, as •well as the nuclei of the enclosing capsule of the cell, have all suffered change by this treatment. The stimulus was applied for only fifteen seconds of each minute, the remaining forty-five seconds being given to rest. In this way the cells here figured had been stimulated over a period of five hours. The nuclei of the sheath are flattened, the cytoplasm of the Fig. SI.— Two sections, A and /;, from the first thoracic spinal ganglion of a cat. B is from the gan- glion which bad been electrically stimulated through its nerve for live hours. A, from the correspond- ing resting ganglion, The shrinkage of the structures connected with the stimulated cells is the most marked general change, n, nucleus; n, s, nuclei of the capsule; v, vacuole; X 500 diameters (Hodge). nerve-cells somewhat shrunken and vacuolated. With osmic acid the nuclei of the stimulated cells stain more darkly and the cytoplasm less darkly than in a resting cell. Tn the nerve-cells the nucleus is shrunken and crenated, and the nucleolus is also diminished in size. In the first experiments the attempt was made to demonstrate a measur- able change within the nerve cell-bodies as the result of stimulation. Assum- ing the nuclei of these cells to be approximately spherical, and calculating their volume as spheres, the shrinkage amounted to that shown in the follow- ing table : CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. 193 Table showing the Decrease in the Volume of the Nucleus of Stimulated Spinal Ganglion-cells of Cats. Stimulation for fifteen seconds alternating with rest for forty -five seconds (Hodge). Stimulation continued for 1 hour. 2.5 hours. 5 10 Shrinkage in the volume of the nuclei of the stimulated cells. 22 per cent. 21 " 24 " 44 " This table further shows that the shrinkage is greater, the greater the time during which the stimulus was applied. There is thus established not only the fact of a change in the cell, but also a relation between the amount of this change and the length of time during which the stimulus was allowed to act. The results when expressed by a curve yield the following : Per cent. 100 \\ / 90 \\ "\ \ 80 \ \ ^,ms « — V- X / 70 60 \ s 50 l 1 Y 1 1 1 1 \ Hours 1 2£ 10 11' 17 23 29 Fig. 82 —The broken line indicates the volume of the nuclei of the spinal ganglion-cells of a cat after stimulation for the times indicated. The solid line indicates the volume of the nuclei, first after severe stimulation for five hours, and then in other cats, also stimulated for five hours, but subsequently allowed to rest for different periods of time. The period of rest is found by subtracting five hours from the time at which the record is made. After twenty-four hours of rest the nucleus is seen to have regained its normal volume (Hodge). Whether these changes could be considered similar to the normal physio- logical variations depended on whether it was possible to demonstrate recovery from them. This was accomplished in the following manner : Under fixed conditions a cat was stimulated in the usual way and the amount of shrinkage in the nuclei of the spinal ganglion-cells was determined. This was found to be almost 50 per cent. Four other cats were similarly treated and then allowed various periods (six and a half, twelve, seventeen, and twenty-four hours) in which to recover. The results appear in Fig. 82. Having thus shown that the change was physiological in the sense thai it was one from which the cells could recover, it remained to be shown that the features of the change were discernible in the living cell, and were not caused secondarily by the actions of the reagents employed in preparing the sections. For the study of' the living cell, frogs were chosen, and the cells of the sympathetic ganglia examined. In these experiments, cells from different frogs were prepared under two different microscopes and kept alive in the Vol. II.— 13 194 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OE PHYSIOLOGY same way by irrigation with a nutrient fluid. In one case, however, the cell was stimulated by electricity, while in the other no stimulation was applied. During the time of the experiment the cell which was nol stimulated re- mained unchanged, while the stimulated cell went through the series of changes exhibited in Fig. 83. l It followed that if these changes were really significant of normal processes they should he found in the nerve-cells of those animals which show well-marked periods of activity, alternating with periods of rest. To determine this, birds and bees were examined, one set of preparations being made from animals which were killed at the beginning of the day, after a night of rest, and the other from those killed at the end of the day, after a period of activity. The cells from the latter animals were found altered in a way similar to that following direct stimulation of the axone. The changes were demonstrated in the cells of the spinal ganglia of English sparrows, of the cerebrum of pigeons and cerebellum of swallows, and of the antennary lobes of hees. These observations therefore support the conclusions drawn from the appearances following direct stimulation. ( >ther observers2 have obtained similar results. The motor cells of the spinal cord and cells of the retina (dogs, Mann) have been added to the list of those showing fatigue changes. In the sympa- thetic ells of the rabbit, both Yas and Mann found, after a short period of stimulation, a pre- liminary swelling of the cell-body, anil the same has been noted by Mann in the case of retinal cells Fig. 83.— Showing the changes \\\ flic dog. in the form <>f tin- nucleus re- rm .. . - , . . , Buitingfromthedirecteiectricai I he application of these observations to changes Btimuiation of the living sym- jn t]1(l human nervous svstem has thus far been pathetic nerve-cell oi a frog. The hour ofobservation is given made only in a casual way, but enough has been within each outline. The ex- already observed to make 'certain that the results perimeul lasted six hours and forty-nine minutes. \ control are applicable. cell treated during this time in u .,, , lJj.1j.j_i i i 1 Ml the same manner! except that U Will be noted that the changes above described it was not stimulated, showed follow variations in the amount of stimulation, the no changes (Hodgi . ... . . , ,. nutrient conditions represented by the surrounding plasma remaining nearly constant. This latter, however, may undergo 1 Hodge: Journal of Morphology, 1892, vol. vii. - Vas: Archiv fur mikroskopische Anatomie, 1892 ; Mann: Journal of Anatomy and Physiology, 1894. CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. 195 alteration, and recent observations show that in various forms of poisoning by inorganic substances or in zymotic diseases the nervous system and espe- cially the cell-bodies are affected early and in a profound manner.1 Fatigue in Nerve-fibres. — There is no evidence for fatigue changes in nerve-fibres. For the full discussion of this question the reader is referred to page 96. Atrophic Influences. — When a nerve-cell is not kept active by the impulses passing within it, it usually atrophies and may degenerate. The reason for this appears to be that the loss of those changes which accompany the nerve-impulses decrease the vigor of the nutritive processes. For the detailed study of metabolic changes within the cell-body the method of Nissl2 has been of prime importance. This method consists in fixing and hardening the nerve-tissue in 96 per cent, alcohol and staining with hot methylene blue. As a result, the cell-bodies especially, retain the stain, and in the cells there is a "stainable substance" characteristically arranged in small masses. For a given animal the arrangement of the " stainable substance" is char- acteristic of the cells from different divisions of the nervous system. In a general way, too, cells occupying homologous positions in the central system of mammals tend to have the substance arranged in a similar manner. But the characteristic picture is modified in any given case by the age of the animal and by the pathological conditions which may have surrounded the cell chosen for study. The changes in the picture may be described as variations in (1) the stainable substance; (2) in the non-stainable fibrillar framework which appears to enclose the former. In both of these, variations may be accompanied by gross physical changes, i. e., alterations in the size of the cell-body, the nucleus and its parts, and alterations in the position of the nucleus, which may appear pushed to the periphery of a swollen cell, or even extruded from it. These physical changes are, of course, the effects of the action of the alcohol and other reagents employed on the cells altered from the normal, and while these physical changes serve most admirably to distinguish the normal from the abnormal cells, they do not necessarily represent the condition of the abnormal cells during life, a cell with an extruded nucleus, for example, being a ease in point. These changes may ultimately cause the death of the element. The stainable substance is found to be extremely sensitive to variations in the physiological conditions surrounding the cell, and therefore to be most important for the revealing of the effect of all sorts of changed conditions, such as starvation, activity, fatigue, injury to the axone, or injury to the afferent neurones bringing impulses to this particular cell, and, finally, the effects of toxins circulating in the blood. ^Schaffer: Ungarisches Arehivfur Medicin, 1893; Pandi: Ibid., 1894; Popoff: \'irrhow's Archiv, 1S94; Tsehistowitsch : Petersburger medicinische Wochenschrift, 1895. 1 The publications of Nissl have not yet been printed in a compact form. The voluminous bibliography of the author is given by Barker : The Nervous System, 1S99, pp. 105, 100. 196 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OE PHYSIOLOGY. Amputation in Man. — When the oerves to a limb have been severed, the consequent changes in the spinal cord depend on the age of the patient at the date of operation, the length of time elapsing between the operation and death, and the level on the limb at which the amputation was made. When the amputation occurs early in life, and the time before death is long, and the level of the amputation high, the alterations are maximum, and consist in an atrophy in the- peripheral efferent nerve-fibres, slight atrophy (or some- times complete disappearance) of the spinal ganglion-cell bodies, atrophy of dorsal root-fibres and their continuations within the cord, and, on the ventral side, disappearance or atrophy of the motor (efferent) cell-bodies in the ventral horn of the cord, together with their axonic outgrowths, the ventral root-fibres, the effect extending outward through the peripheral nerve to the point of section (see Fig. 84). The final appearances are brought about by slow changes, often requiring years for their completion, and hence most of the cases examined tend to show less change than is here described.1 Fig. 84.— Cross-section of the spinal cord of the chick, X 100 diameters (van Uehuchten) ; D, dorsal sur- face ; V, ventral surface ; d. r, dorsal root ; v. r, ventral root; g, spinal ganglion. On the left the arrows indicate the direction of the larger number of impulses in the dorsal and ventral roots respectively. The small arrow on the right dorsal root calls attention to the fact that some axones arising in the ven- tral lamina emerge through the dorsal root and convey impulses in the direction indicated. The disturbance caused in the two sets of cells is, however, not the same. In the case of the cells of the spinal ganglion, the chief pathway by which they are stimulated under normal conditions is so far mutilated that probably only a small number of impulses passes over them. That some do pass is indicated by the sensations apparently coming from the lost limbs — sensations which are often very vivid and minutely localized.2 Thus the cell-bodies located in the spinal cord are to a great degree deprived by such an operation of one principal group of incoming impulses, namely — those which arrive through the dorsal root-fibres that are most closely associated with them ; but at the same time there remain many other ways in which these same cells are normally stimulated. The efferent path- 1 Marinesco: NeuroL Centralbl., 1892 (reviews the literature); Gregoriew : Zeilschrift /. Heillcunde, 1894, T.d. xv. '-' Weir Mitchell : Injuries of Nerves, Philadelphia, 1872. CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. 197 way from these cells is incomplete, and the impulses which must pass along the stumps are inefficient. That impulses do pass along the stumps of the efferent roots is beyond question, since, when the distal portion of an effer- ent nerve is cut off, the cell can be shown to still discharge through the por- tion of the fibres connected with the cell-bodies, and, finally, there is always a tendency for the cut fibre to regenerate, which indicates activity through its entire length. Wherever in the central system a group of fibres forms the chief pathway for the impulses arriving at a given group of cells then the destruction of these afferent fibres brings about the more or less complete atrophy of the cells about which they terminate, and this effect is the more marked the younger the animal at the time of injury. Examples of this relation are found in the behavior, of the nuclei of the sensory cranial nerves. Thus the activity of a given cell contributes to the strength of its own nutritive processes, and different cell-elements, so far as they are physiologi- cally associated, stand in a nutritive or trophic relation to one another such that the receiving cell is in some measure dependent for its nutrition on the cell which stimulates it. Degeneration of Nerve-elements. — -All parts of a nerve-cell are under the control of that portion of the cell-body which contains the nucleus ; in this respect the nerve-elements are similar to other cells which have been studied, and in which the nucleated portion of the cell is found to be alone capable of further growth. It was shown by Waller1 that when sepa- rated from the cell-body of which it wras an outgrowth, a nerve-fibre belong- ing to the peripheral nerve soon degenerate from the point of section to its final distribution. The process is designated as secondary or " Wallerian degeneration." According to recent studies on this subject,2 this degener- ative change occurs practically simultaneously along the entire length of the portion cut off. The changes following the section of medullated nerve- fibres consist in a fragmentation of the axis-cylinder followed by its dis- appearance ; enlargement and multiplication of the nuclei of the medullary sheath, and absorption of the medullary substance, so that in the course of the fibres there remains at the completion of the process the primitive sheaths together with the sheath-nuclei. In the early stages of this process the medullary sheath, moreover, undergoes some changes, the result of which is that it stains more deeply with osmic acid, and hence appears very black in comparison with the normal fibres about it (Marchi). These changes, as shown by the method of Marchi, may follow even slight injuries to the nerve- fibres — such as compression for a short time. Concerning the progress of degenerative changes in the non-mcdiillatcd fibres information is scanty. Bowditch and Warren3 observed that when the sciatic nerve of the cat was sectioned, degeneration of the motor and 1 Nouvelle methode anatomi<]m pour ['investigation du Syateme nerveux, Bonn, 1851. 'Howell and Huber: Journal of Physiology, 1S92, vol. xii. ' Journal of Physiology, 1885, vol. vii. 198 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. vaso-constrictor fibres in the peripheral portion went on at about the same rate. Stimulation of the peripheral part of the nerve gave a vaso-dilator reaction after the vaso-constrictor reaction had entirely disappeared, suggest- ing that the constrictor fibres degenerate more rapidly than do the dilators, although it is not improbable that the dilator fibres in this location really belong to the medullated class (Howell). After five days no vaso-motor reaction at all could be obtained. In a recent study by Tuckett1 of the degeneration of the non-medullated fibres contained in the branches springing from the superior cervical ganglion, it is stated that the degeneration, as traced by histological and physiological methods is complete within thirty to forty hours after section of the fibres, and that the degenerative changes involve only the core of the fibres, the outside sheath and nuclei being un- affected. In the central system, the distal portion of the fibres separated from the cell-bodv degenerate, as at the periphery, and this reaction has therefore formed a means by which to study the architecture of the central system. The details of the process are, however, not clear. Nutritive Control. — So far, then, as the principal outgrowth of the nerve- cell is concerned, it is found to be always under the nutritive control of the cell-body from which it springs. The changes which take place when the spinal roots are cut will serve to illustrate this control (see Fig. 85). Sec- Fig. 85.— Schema of a cross-section of the spinal cord, showing the dorsal and ventral roots and the points at which they may be interrupted: D.r., dorsal root; V.r., ventral root; G, ganglion; M, muscle; S, skin ; 1, lesion between ganglion and cord ; 2, lesion between muscles and cord ; 3, lesion between skin and ganglion; 4, combination of 2 and :;. tion of the dorsal root at the distal side of the spinal ganglion at 3, causes a degeneration of all the fibres which form the dorsal nerve-root distal to the ganglion. Section of the dorsal root at 1, causes degeneration, central to the section, of those nerves which an' outgrowths from the cell-bodies of the spinal ganglion. Section of the ventral root at 2, causes a degeneration distal to the point of section in those fibres which form the ventral root and which arise from the cells within the spinal cord. Tn each case, therefore, the necessary degeneration occurs on the side of the section away from the cell- body. The fraction of the neurone on the other side of the section may also degenerate under certain conditions, but the degeneration is not inevitable.2 'Tuckett: Journal of Physiology, 18%, vol. xix. 2 Kre£in:inn : Arbeiten aua millimeter a day, and hence the return of function tends to be delayed by any increase in the distance between the cut ends of the nerve. It appears also that the return of the cutaneous sensibility is more rapid than the return of motion (Howell and Iluber), from which we infer that the afferent fibres (from the skin) regenerate more rapidly than the efferent fibres to the muscles. Vanlair found that when the regenerated sciatic nerve of a dog was cut a second time, it not only again regenerated, but did so more rapidly than in the first case. Much interest has always attached to the exact course taken by the re- generating fibres. They appear in a general way to be guided by the old sheaths of the peripheral portion. Hut the peripheral nerves contain both afferent and efferent fibres, and it would appear most probable that in the process of reformation the new fibres should undergo much rearrangement. Since the peripheral portion of the nerve acts as a guide to the growing fibres, the experiment has been tried of cross-suturing two mixed nerves. This has been done with the median and ulnar nerves in doers. Reunion of the crossed 'Worcester: Journal of Experimental Medicine, 1S98, vol. iii. p. 597, describes a case of apparent regeneration of a fibre-bundle in the mid-brain, and cites the literature. 1 Journal of Morphology, 1895, vol. xi. s Archives de Physiologie normale et pathologique, 1894. CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. 201 nerves occurred and sensation and motion returned to the affected parts of the limbs.' It is plain thai by this arrangement the skin and muscles at the periphery must have acquired central connections with the spinal cord very different from those normal to them. From the experiments of Cunningham,2 it appears that the results of the cross-suturing of nerve-trunks are about what other facts would lead us to expect. If the trunks concerned control muscles acting in a similar manner, then cross-suturing- produces but slight incoordination as a resull ; where, however, the central trunks normally innervate antagonistic muscles, then incoordination follows and persists. The stimulation of the cerebral cortex at the centre for a given muscle group always causes impulses to pass along the efferent fibres which normally innervate that group, no matter to what muscles these fibres may have been secondarily attached by cross-suturing. More- over, striped muscles which normally exhibit rhythmic contractions lose this function when their innervation is changed by cross-suturing to a nerve-trunk which normally innervates an arhythmic muscle. Thus the central nervous system, in dogs, at least, does not adapt itself to the changed conditions introduced by cross-suturing. In a series of investigations, Lam-lev ! has been able to show that when the preganglionic fibres of the thoracic nerves, which send branches to differ- ent groups of cells in the superior cervical ganglion, are allowed to regenerate after section, the several bundles of fibres appear to find and become attached to the cell-group which they normally controlled, since stimulation of the several roots after regeneration gave the reactions which were characteristic for them. However, there is reason to think that the arrangement after regeneration is not exactly the same as that before, and that some fibres have strayed from their original connections. Further, Langley4 has been able by cross-suturing to establish a connection of the lingual and the vagus nerves respectively with the cervical sympathetic nerve, and so with the superior cervical sympathetic ganglion. Thus we have evidence that fibres other than those normally associated with the ganglion cells can at times form functional connections with them and carry impulses which excite them to their normal functions. This result has an important bearing on the theory of the stimu- lation of one element by another. The reaction following the indirect excita- tion of these cells depends, therefore, on the connections made by their axones, and not on the source of the fibres which excite them. The regeneration thus far described has been that of the axone by the cell-body or perikaryon. Concerning the regeneration of the dendrites, we have no information. The possibility of the formation in mammals of new nerve-cells by the division of nerve-elements which are already mature and have been func- tional, has been claimed. 1 Journal of Physiology, L895, vol. xviii. 1 Cunningham : American Journal of Physiology, 1898, vol. i. 3 Langley : Journal of Physiology, 1897, vol. xxii. p. 215. 4 Langley : Ibid., 1898-9, vol. xxiii. j>. 240. 202 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OE PHYSIOLOGY. Karvokinetic figures in mature nerve-cells utter injury have been demon- strated, but we have yet to learn exactly what cells can exhibit this reaction, and what becomes of them at the end of the nuclear changes. As there is no reason to think that in mammals such a neoformation of neurones in the nervous system lias any significance for the general physiology of the animal, we shall pass the point with a mere reference to the literature.1 PART II.— THE PHYSIOLOGY OF GROUPS OF NERVE-CELLS. A. Architecture and Organization of the Central Nervous System. Since the nerves form the pathways by which the sensory surfaces of the body are put into connection with the central system, and also the pathways by which this system in turn is rendered capable of controlling the tissues of D.C V.R D.P Fig. 86.— Schema of the arrangement of the human spinal cord as seen in cross-section ; for clearness the afferent fibres are shown on the left s i : 1>. I'., dorsal ro"t ; V. /.'., ventral root ; 1>. P., direct pyramidal fibres; C. P., crossed pyramidal films ; C., direct cerebellar tract; .1. L., antero-lateral tract ; D. C, dorsal columns. The various classes nf cell bodies are Indicated by the manner of drawing. expression, it becomes at once important to determine over what nerves the impulses arrive, how they travel through that system, and by what other nerve- they arc again delivered :it the periphery. The arrangement of these paths as found in the adult human nervous system is our principal object; at the same time it should not he forgotten that the reactions of simpler mam- malian systems have furnished the greater number of facts, and to them we must constantly refer. General Arrangement of the Central Nervous System. — As the typical arrangement of the neurones is found in the spinal cord, the schematic representation (Figs. 86, 87) of a cross-section through this part will most readily illustrate it. In accordance with this arrangement of the nervous system, as shown in 1 Tedeschi, A , : .1 natomi&ch-ejcperimentellen Beilrag aim Sludien der Regeneration des Gewebe dcs Centralnervensystems. Bcitriige zur palhologischen Anatomie und zur allegemeinen Pathologie, Jena, 1897, xxi. 43-72, 3 pi. CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. 203 Figs. 86, 87, the elements which compose it fall into three groups: (1) The afferent neurones; those whose function it is to convey impulses due to external stimuli from the periphery, including the muscles and joints, to the central system. The expression "external stimuli" is in this case intended to include beside those outside of the body, also such stimuli as act within the tissues of the body but outside of the central nervous system ; for ex- ample, those acting on tendons and muscles, and affecting the afferent nerves which terminate in them. The dorsal roots of the spinal cord arise from the cell bodies in the spinal ganglion. Sir Charles Bell (1811) showed that these roots are sensory, since in animals stimulation of the central end of the severed root causes reflex movements and ex- pressions of pain, while in man stimulation of these fibres in the stump of an amputated limb may give rise to all the sensa- tions which would be derived from their stimulation in the normal limb. In some vertebrates a few efferent axones leave the spinal cord by the dorsal roots. These fibres can be seen in the chick (Fig. 87). In the frog stimula- tion of the peripheral end of the severed dorsal root may cause contraction of the skeletal mus- cles.1 There is no good evi- dence, however, that these fibres are present in mammals. (2) The ccui ml neurones; those the axones of which never leave the central system, and the function of which is 1<> distribute within this system the impulses which have there been received. (3) The efferent neurones; or those the axones of which pass outside of 1 R. .1. Morton-Smith : Journ. Physiol, vol. xxi. p. L01. Fig. s7.— Schema of the distribution of the efferent fibres of the spinal roots: 4, afferent fibres in the dorsal root only ; /•:, /■.', efferent fibres In both dorsal and ventral roots. In the ventral root one group of efferent tiiires goes to m, the striped muscles; another group to ganglion- cells, S, forming a single sympathetic ganglion, or to n\ cells located In more than one sympathetic ganglion, but all connected with one efferent fibre by means of Its col laterals; P, peripheral plexuses into which the axonea hi Bome sympathetic cells run. 204 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OE PHYSIOLOGY. d.r. the central system, and which carry impulses to the periphery. In this last group, again, two minor divisions may be made, namely : (a) the efferent ele- ments, the cell-bodies of which lie within the central system, as is the ease with those giving rise to the ventral roots ; (b) those forming the peripheral ganglia entirely outside of the central system — the sympathetic ganglia and the more or less solitary cells which take pari in the formation of the peripheral plexuses. It was Sir Charles Bell who also showed the motor character of the ventral roots. Nevertheless the observation was soon made, that while stimulation of the central end of the severed ventral root was always without apparent effect, the stimulation of the peripheral end in addition to the typical motor responses might .sometimes cause expressions of pain. This latter result was obtained even when the mixed nerve trunk, beyond the union of the two roots, had Keen severed, so that the only possible pathway for the impulses was through the junction of the two roots to the spinal ganglion, and so by the dorsal root to the cord. It appears probable from studies on the degeneration of the root fibres that the peripheral axones of some afferent neurones on their way to the meninges do run hack toward the cord within the ventral root, and that it is the stimulation of these fibres which gives rise to the phenomenon of "recurrent sensibility" as it is called. The "afferent neurones" (1) have their cell-bodies collected to form the spinal gan- glia.1 The distal branches of these cells form the peripheral sensory nerves, and the prox- imal branches combine to form the dorsal nerve roots. On entering the walls of the spinal cord these latter fibres divide info two principal longitudinal branches which lie about the dorsal horns and form the major part of the dorsal columns. From time to time the ends of these branches, or their collaterals, enter the gray matter of the cord. Thus, in a cross-section of the cord, the dorsal column and the gray matter represent the localities where the axones of the afferent elements are found. The afferent cranial nerves which can be homologized with the afferent spinal nerves have a corresponding distribution in the bulb. The "efferent neurones" (3) have their cell-bodies only in the ventral horns of the spinal cord, or the homologous localities in the bulb and brain stem. Sec Figs. 88, 89, which show the part of the medullary tube divided 1 Recent work on the spinal ganglia shows that in addition t<> the elements usually deserihed, they probably contain cells, the axones of which are distributed entirely within the spinal gan- glion, and also cells which send their branches to the distal side only of the ganglion. See Dogiel : Anat. Anz. Jena, 1896, Bd. xii. Fig. 88.— Cross-sectioD in the cer- vical region of a fetal human spinal cord at tlir sixth week ; < 50 diameters (V. Kolliker): <\ central canal; o. a., groove separating the two laminae ;d. p., dorsal lamina ; v, p., ventral lamina, in which alone arc Located nerve-cells the axones of which leave the central system; d. r., dorsal root; v. r., ventral root. ( EN Til 1 1 /, NE RVOl rS 8 I rS TEM. •_'< i.j by His into the ventral and dorsal laminae during development. 'Flic ventral horns of the gray substance form pari of the ventral laminae. The cells of the sympathetic system which are interpolated in one portion of the pathway formed by the efferent elements lie, of course, entirely out- side of the central system. (See Fig. 87.) The central neurone- (2) occupy all parts of the central system, and hence where the bodies or branches of the first two groups are absent, the system is composed of central neurone- only. Arrangement of the Cells Forming- the Several Groups. — All three groups of elements are grossly arranged so as to be bilaterally symmetrical with reference to the dorso-ventral median plane of the body. There are some minor exceptions to this general statement, but these are not known to have any physiological significance. Fig. 89.— Schema showing the encephalon and cord; the unshaded portion is that derived from the dorsal lamina; the shaded that from the ventral (from Mi not) : C, cerebrum ; Cb, cerebellum ; F, foramen of Monro; /, infundibulum ; .1/, bull.; 0, olfactory lube; P, pons; Q, quadrigemina ; Sp.c, spinal cord; ///, third ventricle; IV, fourth ventricle. The main axones of the afferent elements are distributed almost entirely to the dorsal columns, and to the gray matter of that side of the cord on which they enter, though some crossing occurs in the dorsal commissure. In the cord, the efferent elements have their cell-bodies mainly on the side ol the cord from which the efferent fibres emerge. In the case of some cranial efferent nerves the arrangement is different. There is found, for instance, a partial decussation of the fibre- of the oculo-motorius ; complete decussation in the case of the patheticus, and no decussation in the case of the abducens. It is the central cells which furnish almost all the axones forming the com- missures, the decussating bundles ami the projection systems, while the association tracts arise entirely from them. Segmentation. — The grouping of the cell-bodies of the afferent fibres i- originally segmental, one spinal ganglion corresponding to each segment of the trunk. Tn the brain, the original segmental arrangement has been greatly modified. In the trunk, too, the distribution of the distal portion of 206 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. the afferent nerve- Is segmental, the area of skin involved forming a band about the body. ( >n the other hand, the distribution of the proximal branches forming the dorsal roots is such that while part of the axones and their collaterals establish connection with the cord and bulb near the level at which the axone joins it, the principal divisions of the axone often pass along the cord a greater or Less distance in both directions, and thus a long stretch of the cord may receive impulses by way of a single afferent element. In some of the lower vertebrates the arrangement of the "efferent" cells is plainly segmental, but in man and the higher mammals this is hardly to be demonstrated. In the least modified parts of the cord, the efferent fibres do arise from cell-bodies mainly within the segment from which the ventral root emerges. But this massing of the efferent cell-bodies is largely obscured by the presence of central cells through the entire length of the ventral horns, while in the portions of the cord controlling the limbs the columns of cells furnishing fibres to a given ventral root may extend through as many as three segments of the cord. The distribution of the efferent fibres is evidently segmental in plan, though highly modified everywhere except in the thoracic cord supplying the portion of the trunk between the limbs. The principal peculiarity in the group of central cells is the great increase in the mass of them as we pass from the cord cephalad, the cerebrum, for example, being composed entirely of central cells. Relative Development of Different Parts. — The bulk of the three subdivisions which have been named is very unequal. The central system is far more massive than the afferent and efferent taken together, but the relation cannot be stated with any exactness, since the mass of the peripheral system is not definitely known. Connections between Cells. — In determining the connection between cells which permits a nerve impulse in one cell to stimulate another, the fact that the axone is the outgrowth of a cell-body, and that each cell is an inde- pendent morphological unit, forms the point of departure. Under these circumstances the question of the connection between cells takes the more explicit form of the question whether cell-branches may become continuous by secondary union. In several vertebrates there is good histological evidence that such secondary union occurs in a few eases in the central system. In one type the axone of one element spreads out and encloses the cell- body of a second after the manner of a cup holding a ball. In other cases it appears that the terminals of a given axone may even penetrate the cell substance of the receiving neurone. These an' examples of concrescence. In the majority of eases, however, a close approximation of the parts of two nerve-cells is alone to be seen I Fig. 90). The termination of the discharging axone may be by fibrils or expanded di>ks, and occur either close to or upon the body, dendrites, or even collaterals' of the receiving neurone. If, as seems probable, the dendrites form an important pathway by which the receiving neurone is 1 Held: Arehivf. Anat. a. Physiol., Anat. Abthl., Leipzig, 1897. CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. 207 excited, then a cell with many dendrites should offer more receiving points than one with few. It is perfectly evident, however, that in many cases the dendrites are not the only pathway by which impulses may travel toward the cell-body. Theories of the Passage of the Nerve-Impulses. — Accepting the view- that, with the exceptions just noted, the nervous system is composed of dis- continuous but closely approximated cell-elements, it remains to explain how impulses arising within the limits of one element are able to influence others. The relation between two neurones is quite comparable to that between a muscle and the nerve-fibres controlling it, but the recognition of that fact does not afford us much assistance. As an hypothesis the passage of the stimulus may be assumed to depend on chemical changes set up at the tips of the terminals and affect- ing the surrounding substance, which, thus affected, acts to stimulate some point on the wall of the neighboring cell, either along a dendrite or on the cell-body itself. The suggestion has been made that in some cases the space between two neurones may be varied by amoeboid changes in the dendrites and terminals of the elements concerned. Although much may be said a priori in favor of this hypothesis, good histological evi- dence is still wanting. The structural changes which permit the stimulation of one element to affect another are completed slowly, and, as we shall later see, these changes continue in some parts of the human nervous system up to middle life. From what has just been stated, it follows that the nervous system of the immature person is quite a different thing from that of one mature, since in the former it is more schematic, more simple, the details of the pathways not- having been as yet filled out. Moreover, considering the slow and minute manner in which the central system is organized by the growth of the cell- branches, it is the list place where there should be expected structural uni- formity in the details of arrangement. B. Reflex Action. Conditions of Stimulation. — The conditions necessary for the generation of a nerve impulse are an external stimulus acting on an irritable neurone. While life exists, stimulation of varying intensity is always going on, and Fig. 90. — Showing at the lower edge of the figure a series of basket-like terminations of axones which surround the bodies of the great cells of Purkinje in the cortex of the cerebellum (Ramon y Cajal) : C, cell-body; N, axones; B, basket-like terminations arising from cell C, and enclosing the cells of Purkinje. 208 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. hence there is no iiMiim in at which the nervous system is not stimulated, and no moment at which the effectiveness of this stimulus is not varied. The response t<> this continuous and ever-varying stimulation i- not necessarily always evident, but occasionally intensification of the stimuli renders them so Strong that an evident reaction follows. Though the foregoing statements suggest that the chief variable is that represented by the stimulus, the strength of which changes, yet as a matter of fad the variations in the physiological (chemical) condition of the nerve-cells are equally important; but neither factor can be studied independently. The term "central stimulation" has been sometimes employed. For ex- ample, the spasmodic movements of the young child, when there is no change noticeable in the external stimuli acting upon it, are sometimes attributed to this cause ; but these, although doubtless due to central changes, altering the irritability of the cells, are most properly classed with the reactions which follow the external stimulus. The misconceptions here to be avoided are those of supposing that the nervous system is at any time unstimulated, and that the evident responses follow a change of the external stimulus only. When the impulse in one cell-element is used to arouse an impulse in another, as in all experiments where the nerve-cells are examined in a physi- ological series, the strength of the impulse from the second is not easily pre- dicted. This is explained as due to variations in the ease with which the impulse in one element stimulates the next, and also to the variations in the second cell of those conditions which determine the intensity with which it shall discharge. When an impulse has once entered the central system by way of a dorsal nerve root, it is found to follow the course of the afferent axones within the central system, and thus must be distributed almost simultaneously to a length of cord coextensive with that of the branches of the afferent axones. The arrangement makes possible the stimulation of a large number of central cells, and thus greatly increases the distribution of the initial disturb- ance. In the case of some of the cells about which the branches of the axone end, the impulse will not be adequate to cause in them a discharge, although it may still produce a certain amount of chemical change in them. The im- pulse thus tends to disappear within the system by producing, in part, chemi- cal changes strong enough to cause a discharge of the next clement in the series and. in an increasing number, similar changes of a less intensity. Diffusion of Central Impulses. — If the previous description has been correct, two very important events occur: in the firsl place, the impulse reaches a far greater number of cell- than evidently discharge, and in the second, the pathway followed by the impulses which do produce the discharge is by no means the only pathway over which the impulses can or do travel. Simple Reflex Actions. — We turn next to an examination of these groups of neurones in action. 'flic simplest and most constant of the co-ordinated reactions of the nerv- CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. 209 ous system are termed reflex. The term involves the idea thai the response is not accompanied by consciousness, and is dependent on anatomical condi- tions in the central system which are only in a slight degree subject to physi- ological modifications. This view of reflex activities is in a large measure justified by the facts, but at the same time it must he held subject to many modifications, and it is not possible to make a hard and fast line between reflex and voluntary reactions. The principal features of a reflex act may be illustrated by following a typical experiment : If the central nervous system of a frog be severed at the bulb, so as to separate from the spinal cord all the portions of the central system above it, and the brain be destroyed, the animal is for a time in a condition of collapse. If, after recovering from the immediate shock, such a frog be suspended by the lip, it will remain motionless, the fore legs extended and the hind legs pendent, though very slightly flexed. If such a frog were dissected down to the nervous system, there would be found the following arrangement : Afferent fibres running from the skin, muscles, and tendons, and entering the cord by way of the dorsal nerve-roots. The central mass of the spinal cord itself in which these roots end, each root marking the middle of a segment. Within the cord and stretching its entire length are to be found the central cells, interpolated more or less numerously between the terminals of the afferent neurones and the cell-bodies of the efferent neurones. From each segment of the cord go the ventral root-fibres passing in part to the muscles and in part to the ganglia of the sympathetic system. The mechanism demanded for a reflex response is an afferent path leading to the cord ; cells in the cord by which the incoming impulses shall be there distributed; and a third set of efferent elements to carry the outgoing impulses to the1 terminal organ which gives the response. It is important to consider in detail what occurs in each portion of this reflex arc. In a frog thus prepared, stimulation of the skin in any part supplied by the sensory nerves originating from the spinal cord causes a contraction of some muscles. Influence of Location of Stimulus. — The muscles which thus contract tend to be those innervated from the same segments of the cord which receive the sensory nerves that have been stimulated. Thus stimulation of the skin of the breast causes movements of the fore limbs, and stimulation of the rump or legs corresponding movements of the hind limbs. It is noticeable, how- ever, that wherever the stimulus i> applied the hind Limbs have a tendency to move at tlie same time that the muscles most directly concerned contract, [f the attempt is made to correlate these variations in reaction with varia- tions in the structure of the cord, we have to picture the simplest reactions (from the same level) as dependent on the formation of terminals on the afferent fibre just after its entrance into the cord and in the immediate neigh- borhood of an efferent neurone. In the second case either the afferent axone i- extended some distance through the cord forming several terminations by Vol. ir. -u 210 AN AMERICAN TEXT- BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. its collaterals, or a central cell is excited and serves to carry the impulse to a distance. Segmental Reactions. — In attempting to explain this associated con- traction of the leg muscles, it must he remembered that the hind legs are. •par excellence, the motile extremities of the frog, and therefore all general movements involve their use. We inter from this, moreover, that the arrange- ment in the spinal cord of the frog is not such that the sensory impulses com- ing into any segment tend to rouse exclusively the muscles innervated by that segment, but that these incoming impulses are diffused in the cord unevenly and in such a way as to easily involve the segments controlling the legs. As reflex co-ordinating centres, therefore, the several segments of the cord have not an equal value. When the stimulus is applied on one side of the median plane, the responses firs! appear in the muscles of the same side; and if the stimulus is slight they may appear on that side only. The incoming impulses arc therefore first and mosl effectively distributed to the efferent cells located on the same side of the cord as that on which these impulses enter. Such a statement is most true, however, when the stimulus enters the cord at the level where the nerves to the limbs are given off. At other levels the diffusion to the limb centres may take place more readily than to the cells in the opposite half of the same segment. When the muscles on the side opposite to the point of stimulation contract it is found 'hat they correspond to the group of muscles giving the initial response on the side of the stimulus. The diffusion then tends to cross the cord and to involve the cells located at the same level as that at which the incoming impulses enter it. There is some reason to think that when the impulses enter the cord toward the lumbar end the path by which the diffusion takes place with least resistance is not the shortest one between the two groups of cells, but a path toward the cephalic end of the cord, so that the impulses tend to pass up the cord on one side and down on the other.1 Strength of Stimulus. — In a reflex response the strength of the stimulus influences the extent to which the muscles are contracted, the number of muscles taking part in the contraction, and the length of time during which the contraction continues. That the strength of the stimulus influences the extent to which the contraction of a given group of muscles takes place is easily shown when, for example, the toe of a reflex frog which has been sus- pended i> stimulated by pinching it or dipping it in dilute acid. In this case, if the stimulus hi' slight, the leg is hut slightly raised, whereas, if the stimu- lus he strong, it is drawn up high. In the same way by altering the stimulus the muscles which enter into the contraction may he only those controlling the joints of the foot, whereas, with stronger stimuli, those for the knee and hip are successively affected, thereby involving a much larger number of muscles. Here, too, we infer a spread of the incoming impulses which is orderly, since the several joints of the limb are moved in regular sequence. 1 Rosenthal and Mendelssohn : Neurologisches Centralblatt, 1897, Bd. xvi. S. 978. CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. 211 The responses which are thus obtained arc not spasmodic, but are con- tractions of muscles in regular series, giving the appearance of a carefully co- ordinated movement — a movement that is modified in accordance both with the strength of the stimulus and its point of application. Moreover, such a move- ment may occur not only once, but a number of times, the leg being alter- nately flexed and extended during an interval of several seconds, although the stimulus is simple and of much shorter duration. Continuance of Response. — The continuance of the response after the stimulus has been withdrawn must be, of course, the result of a long-continued chemical change at some point in the pathway of the impulse, and it appears probable by analogy with the results obtained from the direct stimulation of the central cortex, or the spinal cord, that in these cases the stimulating changes are taking place (p. 188) in the central cells or efferent cells ' as well as in the skin supplied by the afferent nerves. Latent Period. — It has been observed that in the case of a reflex frog — that is, a frog prepared as described above, with the spinal cord separated from the brain — an interval of varying length elapses between the application of a stimulus and the appearance of a reaction. The modifications of the interval according to variations in the stimulus have been carefully studied. When dilute acid applied to the skin is used as a stimulus, this latent interval decreases as the strength of the acid is increased. When separate electrical or mechanical stimuli are employed, the reaction tends to occur after a given number of stimuli have been applied, although the time intervals between the individual stimuli may be varied within wide limits. The experimental evidence for electrical stimuli shows that the time intervals may range between 0.05 second and 0.4 second,2 while the number of stimuli required to produce a response remains practically constant. Summation of Stimuli. — A single stimulus very rarely if ever calls forth a reaction if the time during which it acts is very short, and hence there has developed the idea of the summation of stimuli, implying at some part of the pathway a piling up of the effects of the separately inefficient stimuli to a point at which they ultimately become effective.3 The details of the changes involved in this summation and the place at which the changes occur, are both obscure, but it would seem most probable that summation is an expression of changes in the relations between the final twigs of the afferent elements and the cell-bodies of the central or efferent elements, which permit the better passage of the impulse from one element to the other, for the evidence strongly indicates that the course of the impulse can be interrupted at these junctions. The foregoing paragraphs have been concerned mainly with changes occurring in the afferent portion- of the pathway. Next to be considered is 1 Birge: Arehivfur Anatomic und Physiologie (Physiol. A.bthl>), 1882,8. 484. 2 Ward : Ibid,, 1880. s Gad uud Goldscheider : " Ueber die Summation von Hautreizen," ZeU&chrift Jur klinische Medicin, 1893, Bd. xx. Befte 4-ti. 212 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. the amount of central nervous matter which must be present in the frog's spinal cord in order that the reactions can take place. Reactions from Fractions of the Cord. — -If the construction of the cord was strictly segmental (a condition nearly approached in some worms and arthropods), in the sense thai each segment contained the associated nerves for a given band of skin and muscle, there should be no disturbance on dividing the cord into its anatomical segments; and practically among the invertebrates, where the ganglionic chain is thus arranged, the single segments can perform alone all the reactions of* which they are capable under normal conditions.1 In such invertebrates the only change effected by the combina- tion of the segments is that of co-ordinating in time and in intensity the reactions of the series. If, on the other hand, the segments of the cord were more or less dependent upon one another, and not physiologically equivalent, modifications of various degrees would arise according to the segments isolated. It has been found that the spinal cord of the frog may, under special condi- tions, be reduced to three segments and reactions still be obtained. During the breeding season the male frog, by means of his fore legs, clasps the female vigorously and often for days. If, at this season, there is cut out from the male the region of the shoulder girdle bearing the fore limbs together with the connected skin and muscles, and the three upper segments of the spinal cord, then an irritation of the skin will cause a reflex clasping move- ment similar to that characteristic for the normal male at this season. Reactions in Other Vertebrates. — It must not be thought, howrever, because it is the custom to emphasize the reflex activities of the lower verte- brates, and to show that these reflexes can be carried out even by fractions of the spinal cord alone, that, therefore, the spinal cord is particularly well developed in them. Comparative anatomy shows in the lower vertebrates a simplicity in the structure of the cord quite comparable with that found in the brain, and, as we ascend the vertebrate series, both parts of the central system increase in complexity. In this increase, however, the cephalic divi- sion takes the lead ; and further, by means of the fibre-tracts from it to the cord, the cell-groups in the cord are more and more brought under the influence of the special sense-organs which connect with the encephalon. The physio- logical reactions of the higher vertebrates are especially modified by this hitter arrangement. It is, therefore, true that the cord, as well as the brain, is in man more complicated anatomically than in any of the lower forms, and this is true in spite of the fact that the independent reactions of the human cord are so imperfect. When an amphioxus is cut into two pieces and then put back in the water, a slight dermal stimulus cause- in both of them locomotor movements, such as are made by the entire animal. When a shark (Scyllium canicula) is beheaded, the torso swims in a co-or- dinated manner when returned to the water. Separation of the cord from the 1 Loeb: Einleilung in dit vergleichen.de Gehirnphysiologie und vergleichende Psychologie, Leip- zig, 1899. 3Se"S G | p -■ =. O c 3 P »i 0 _ ?. m Ei 5 ' — i - ^ ~. a it B Fig. 92.— Diagrammatic representation of the lower portion of the human bulb and spinal cord. The cord is divided into Lts four regions : 1, medulla cervicalis; _'. medulla dorsalis : 3, medulla lum- balis; 1, medulla sacralis. Within each region the spinal segments bear Roman numbers. On the left side of the diagram the locality supplied by the sensory (afferent) neurones is indicated by one or more words and these latter are connected with the bulb or the segments of the cord at tin- levels at which the nerves enter. The afferent character is indicated by the arrow-tip on the lines of referenci On the right-hand side the names of muscles or groups of muscles are given, and to them arc drawn reference lines which start from the segments of the cord in which the cell-bodies of origin have been Located. Within the cord itself, the designations for several reflex centres are inscribed in the segment where the mechanism is localized. For example. Reflexus scapularis. Centrum eilio-spinale, Reflexus epigas- tricus, Reflexus abdominalis. Reflexu> cremastericus, Reflexus patellaris, Reflexus tendo Achillis, Cen- trum vesieale, Centrum analc (the last two on the left side of the diagram). (From Iccrnes yrtnoloyicx, Striimpell and Jakob.) Acusticus Vestibularis Gustus Pharynx Oesophagus Larynx, Trachea ,f*tedulkj cervfcaDs 1 ' .',, (Thorax. Abdomen) Regio occipitalis Regio colli Regio nuchas Regio humeri Regio Nervi radialis Regio N. mediani Regio N. ufnaris in Muscuii faciei Mm. pharyngis. palati Mm. laryngis Mm. linguae Oesophagus Sternocleidomastoideus Muscuii colli et nuchae Cucullaris Rhomboidei Diaphragma DeltoirJeus, Supinator lonps Biceps Mil dulla dorsalls Abdomen Thorax [pigastriur Umbilicus Regio glutaealis Regio inguinalis IV M vn Mil Reflex epigastr XI WtexaWommafe Ml Pectoralis major (portio ciavicui.) Teres minor ( "== Pronatores. Brachialis internus \ ^ Triceps Extensores carpi et digitor. longi j % Pectoralis major (portio costalis] g_ Latissimus dorsi, teres major Flexores carpi et digitor. longi) Interossei lumbricales Thenar Hypothenar If lleopsoas IS Regio femoris Regio cruris Vesica Rectum Anus Quadriceps femoris Glulaei, tensor fasc. lat. Adductores femoris Abductores femoris Tibialis anticus Gastrocnemius, Soleus Biceps Semitendi Semimembranosus Flexores pedis Extensores digitorum Peronei Flexores digitorum Mm. vesicates Mm. rectales CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. 213 brain does not deprive a ray (Torpedo oculata) of the power of perfect loco- motion. The same is true of the ganoid fish. In the case of the cyclostome fish (Petromyzon) the beheaded trunk is, in the water, inactive, yet, on gentle mechanical stimulation, it makes inco-ordinated responses ; but, put in a bath formed by a 3 per cent, solution of picro-sulphuric acid, locomotion under the influence of this strong and extensive dermal stimulus is completely per- formed. In the case of the eel the responsiveness even to the picro-sulphuric acid bath is evident in the caudal part of the body alone. 1 n the bony fish this capability of the spinal cord to control locomotion has not been observed.1 In these experiments the central system is represented by the entire spinal cord with the associated nerves, or by some fraction of it; but so simple, constant, and independent are the reactions of the cord under normal condi- tions that a strong stimulus is able to elicit the characteristic responses from even a fragment of the system. The higher Ave ascend in the vertebrate series the less evident do the independent powers of the cord become. Tarchanow 2 has shown that beheaded ducks can still swim and fly in a co-ordinated manner, and among mammals (dog and rabbit) Goltz and Ewald 3 and others have demonstrated that if the lumbar region be separated from the rest of the cord by a cut and the animal allowed to recover from the opera- tion it will with proper care live for many months, and not only are the legs responsive to stimulation of the skin, but the reflexes of defecation and urina- tion are easily induced by slight extra stimulation. An instructive reaction occurs when such an animal is held up so that the hind legs hang free. When thus held, the legs slowly extend by their own weight and then are flexed together. The reaction becomes rhythmic and may continue for a long time. It is assumed in this case that the stretching of the skin and tendons due to the weight of the pendent legs acts as a stimulus, and in consequence the legs are flexed. This act in turn removes the stimulus, and as a result they extend again, to be once more stimulated and drawn up. In man, as a rule, death rapidly follows the complete separation of any considerable portion of the cord from the rest of the central system, especially if the separation be sudden, as in the case of a wound. But Gerhardt4 has recorded the retention of the reflexes in a case of compression of the cord by a tumor, the case having been under observation for four and a half years ; and Hitzig,5 a case in which a total separation between the last cervical and first thoracic segments had been survived for as long as seven years. The principal reaction to be observed in such cases is a contraction of the limb muscles in response to stimulation of the skin, such as a drawing up of the legs when the soles of the feet are tickled. No elaborate reflexes are, how- ever, retained such as would be necessary in acts of locomotion. 1 Steiner : Die Functional des Centralnervensystems mul ihre Phylogeneae, 2te Aluli., "Die Fische," 1888. 2 Tarchanow : Pfl.ugei?8 Archiv, 1885, I'd xxxiii. 3 Goltz und Ewald : Ibid., 1896, Bd. lxiii. 4 Neurologisches Centrcdblatt, 1894, 8. 502. ' Loc. cit. 214 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. It thus appears that the reflex responses, namely, simple reactions unac- companied by consciousness, are in man mainly given by the unstriped muscle-tissue and by glands, and only in a minor degree by the striped muscles. Moreover, while the typical reflex is a reaction over which we cannot exercise direct control, the normal individual has some power over many of these reactions; tor example, the impulse to micturition or defeca- tion can be thus delayed, respiration arrested, and, in some instances, so remote a reaction as the beat of the heart either accelerated or slowed at will. It is of interest to note that many reflexes which in the young are not controlled, as micturition, for instance, become so gradually, a change most probably dependent on the growth of axones from the cephalic centres into the cord, thus subjecting the cord-cells to a new set of impulses which modify their reactions. That such is the case is indicated by the fact that extreme fright or anaesthetics, which diminish the activities of the higher centres, often cause these reactions to take place involuntarily. Other reflexes are present in early life, but disappear later; such are the sucking reflex of the infant, and the remarkable clinging power of the hands, by which a young child is enabled to hang from a liar, thus supporting the weight of its entire body, often for several minutes. This last capacity soon begins to wane, and usually disappears by the second month of life.1 Co-ordination of the Efferent Impulses. — Incessantly the efferent im- pulses pass out from the cord to the muscles and glands. With each fresh afferent impulse those which go out are modified in strength and in their order, but just how they shall be co-ordinated is dependent on so many and such delicate conditions that even in the simplest case the results are to be predicted only in a general way. The attempt to determine the spread of the impulse in the cord by observing the order in which the various muscles of the thigh and leg con- tract in response to thermal stimuli was made by Lombard.2 In a reflex frog the tendons of the leg and thigh muscles were exposed at the knee, and each attached to n writing-rod in so ingenious a manner that simultaneous records of fifteen muscles could sometimes be obtained. The stimulus was a metal tube, filled with water at 47°-61° C, which was applied to the skin. Under these conditions, it was remarkable that a continuous stimulus was often followed, not by a single contraction of the muscles, but by a series of contractions, suggesting that in the central system the cell- were roused to a discharge and then for a time concerned with the preparation for sending out new impulses, and that during this latter period the muscles were relaxed. Apparently a high degree of uniformity in the conditions was obtained in these experiments, but at the same time the reactions were far from uniform, in either the latent time of contraction or the order in which the contraction of the several muscles followed, although certain muscles tended to contract first, and certain series of contractions to reappear. The co-ordination of the 1 Kobinson : Nineteenth Century, 1891. 1 Archiv Jiir Anatomie und Fhysiologie, 1885. CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. 215 contractions is therefore variable in time, even under these conditions. These variations are probably due either to the fact that the impulses arc not dis- tributed in the centre in the same manner on each occasion ; or if they are thus distributed, the central and efferent cells vary from moment to moment in their responsiveness. That these cells should so vary is easy to compre- hend, for all the cell-elements in such a reflex frog are slowly dying. In this process they are undergoing a destructive chemical change, and with these destructive changes are generated weak impulses sufficient to cause their physiological status continually to vary, thus modifying the effects of any special set of incoming impulses acting upon them. It is not to be overlooked also that the dissection of the muscles tested, and the removal of the skin about them, deprived the spinal cord of the incoming impulses due to the stretching of the skin by the swelling of the contracting muscles and disturbed the order and intensity of such sensory impulses as come in from the tendons and the muscles themselves. The observations of both Bickel x and Hering 2 show that these impulses are not necessary for accurate reflex movements of the frog's leg, and thus weaken the force of the suggestion just made. However much these impulses may add to the regularity of the muscular responses, Lombard concludes that the discharge of one efferent cell is not necessary in order that another efferent cell may discharge, but that each discharging cell stands at the end of a physiological pathway and may react independently. Purposeful Character of Responses. — When the muscular responses of a reflex frog to a dermal stimulus are studied, they are seen to have a pur- poseful character, in that they are often directed to the removal of irritation. This is demonstrated by placing upon the skin on one side of the rump a small square of paper moistened with dilute acid. As a result, the foot of the same side is raised and the attempt made to brush the paper away ; if the first attempt fails, it may be several times repeated. When the irritation has been removed the frog usually becomes quiet. If the leg of the same side be held fast after the application of the stimulus, or if the first movements fail to brush away the acid paper, then the leg of the opposite side may be contracted and appropriate movements be made by it. Emphasis has been laid by various physiologists upon reactions of this sort as showing a capa- bility of choice on the part of the spinal cord, thus granting to the cord psychical powers. Against such a view it must be urged that the movements of the leg on the side opposite to the stimulus do not occur until after the muscles of the leg on the same side have responded. When these responses are inefficient because the leg is prevented from moving or because tiny tail to remove the stimulus, the prime fact remains that the stimulus continues /<> ad and the diffusion of the impulses in the cord goes on, involving in either case the nerve-cells controlling the muscles of the opposite leg. The adjust- ment of the reaction of the leg, on whichever side it occurs, is, however, far 1 Bickel : Pfluger'a Archiv, Kd. lxvii. 1 Hering: Archiv fiir experimenleUe Paihologie und Pharmakologie, Bd. xxxviii. 216 AN AMERICAN TEXT- BO OK OF PHYSIOLOGY. from precise ; and although the movements of the leg, when the stimulus i.- applied far up on the rump, differ from those which follow the application of the stimulus to the lower part of the thigh, yet in either case they are very wide, and in both cases the fool is brushed across a large part of both the rump and leg. Considering, therefore, the rather general character of these movements, and the fact that the movements of the opposite leg only follow- after a continued stimulus to the leg of the same side has produced an inef- fective response, it is besl to explain the result by the diffusion of the impulses within the cord, leaving quite to one side the psychical clement. Such reflex actions are in a high degree predictable, but in reality this has little signifi- cance, since there i> but one general movement that a frog in such a condition can make, whether the stimulus be applied to the toes or the rump— namely, the flexion of' the leg — so that under these circumstances the prediction of the kind of movement is a simple matter. The extent of the contraction is related to the intensity of the stimulus, and is in turn dependent on the excitability of the central system, which can be increased or diminished in various ways. The mollification of the reaction as dependent on the location of the stimulus can be in a measure predicted, but the modification is wanting in precision just in so far as the movements themselves are wanting in this quality. Reflexes in Man. — In the normal individual reflexes involving striped muscles are found in the tendon reflexes, of which the knee-kick is an exam- ple, in winking, and the whole series of reflex modifications of respiration, such as coughing, sneezing, and the like. The activities of the alimentary tract are examples of reflex actions involv- ing the contraction of muscles in deglutition, defecation, and similar peristaltic movements in other hollow viscera. These muscle-fibres are for the most part unstriped. So, too, micturition, the cremaster reflex, emission and vaginal peristalsis, and the reactions of parturition are to be classed here. Moreover, the entire vascular system is controlled in this manner, the contraction and distention of the small arteries being in a large measure in response to stimuli originating at a distance ; while as a third group, we have the glands, the activity of which is almost entirely reflex. For the discussion of the various reflexes mediated by the cranial nerves, the reader is referred to the special sections dealing with the cardiac, vasomotor, and respiratory centres in the bulb and the pupillary centres in the mid-brain. Periodic Reflexes. — Not all reflexes are to be obtained from the same animal with equal intensity at all times. In general, frogs in the spring-time and in early summer, after reviving from their winter sleep, are highly irregular in their reflex responses — so irregular that students are advised not to attempt the study of these reaction- at this season. On the other hand, it is during the spring that mating occurs, and during this period the male clasps the female and exhibits the peculiar reflex which has already been described. Comparable with this variation in the frog must be the changes which occur in the spinal cords of migratory birds, which, both in the spring and in the fall, are capable of such extended flights, or in the CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. 217 system of hibernating animals and all animals exhibiting well-marked periodic variations in their habits of life. Variations in Diffusibility. — The degree in which any set of incoming impulses is diffused and modifies the responsiveness of the central system depends, in the first instance, on the physiological connections of the fibres by which they travel, and, in the second, on the particular condition in which the central cells happen to be found. It is observed that by means of drugs it is possible to alter the diffusibility of incoming stimuli to an enormous extent. Strychnin and drugs with a similar physiological action have this as one of their effects. Influence of Strychnin. — The experimental study of strychnin-poisoning shows the following relations : A frog poisoned by the injection of this drug is easily thrown into tetanus whether the brain is intact or has been removed previous to the injection. The drug is found to have accumulated in the substance of the spinal cord.1 The peculiar change wrought in the nervous system is such that a slight stimulus will cause an extended and prolonged tetanic contraction of the skeletal muscles — i. e., the diffusion of impulses within the cord is very wide and (efficient to an unusual degree. The direct application of strychnin to the spinal cord has been carefully studied by Houghton and Muirhead.2 AVhen the strychnin solution was applied locally to the brachial enlargement of the spinal cord of a brainless frog, a subse- quent stimulation of the skin of the arms produced tetanic contractions of the arms, and later, after the poison had acted for a time, of the entire trunk and legs. On the other hand, stimulation of the legs in such a case produced a slight reflex or none at all. Since, in order to cause contraction Df the leg- muscles, the efferent cells controlling the muscles of the leg must, be discharged — and in the one case when the stimulus was applied to the arm region these cells discharged so as to cause a tetanic spasm, while in the other, when the stimulus was applied to the legs, they discharged only slightly — the alteration in the cord produced by the drug must affect some other group than these efferent cells. Since, moreover, a tetanus of the legs could be caused by the stimulation of the skin of the arm, the application of the drug being to the brachial enlargement only, it appears that the central cells, or those conduct- ing the impulses entering by the dorsal root-fibres in the brachial region to the nuclei of the lumbar enlargement, are probably affected ; and. further, that it is on the bodies of these cells that the drug must act. since they alone were in the locality at which the drug was applied. The application of the drug to the dorsal root-ganglia and to the nerve-roots between the ganglia and the cord proved to be without effect, SO that the two parts which can possibly be influenced are the terminations of the sensory afferent nerves within the cord ami some portion (the dendrites ?) of the central cells with which these terminations are associated. lint whether the change is in both these structures or only in one cannot now be determined. 1 Lovett : Journal <;/' Physiology, L888, vol. ix. ': Mrtliral News, June 1, 1S95. 218 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. The diffusion of impulses in the central system depends anatomically not only on the amount of branching among the axones of the individual central cells, but also on the association of many cells together, so as to accomplish a wide distribution of the impulses. In the case of the afferent elements, :i- we have seen, the diffusion depends on the branching of the axones alone. Peripheral Diffusion. — Turning next to the efferent system, we find the conditions for diffusion dependent on the arrangement of several cells in series. When a group of efferent cells discharges, we know from the arrangement of the ventral roots that the impulses leave the cord mainly along the fibres which comprise these roots; hut where the lateral root is presenl they may also pass out over it, as well as over the few efferent fibres found in the dorsal roots. These axones carrying the outgoing impulses have two destinations: (1) The voluntary or striped muscle-fibres; (2) the sympathetic nerve-cells, grouped in masses to form the vagrant ganglia (see Fig. 93). When the impulses are thus sent out there is in the case of motor nerves no diffusion, the effect being limited to the peripheral distribution of the efferent axones, by way of which the impulses leave the central system. The fibres going to the voluntary muscles form, however, but one portion, which has just been indicated as group (1). The connections of the remain- ing group (:2), passing to the sympathetic ganglia, are still to be examined. Sympathetic System. — Associated with the cerebro-spinal system by the efferent axones, and by these alone, is the series of vagrant ganglia and also of peripheral plexuses containing ganglion-cells, which taken together form the sympathetic system.1 This system is composed of neurones always monaxonic, but sometimes with, and sometimes without well-marked den- drites. The cells are more or less grouped in ganglia, and these ganglia interpolated between the efferent axones of the spinal nerve-roots on the one hand and the peripheral plexuses or terminal tissues on the other. The number of cells in the ganglia is greater than the number of spinal root axones going to them, and hence their interpolation in the course of the ventral root- fibres increases the number of pathways toward the periphery, as is shown in Fig. 93. In speaking of the fibres concerned, it is desirable to distinguish between the pre-ganglionic, or those originating in the medullary centres and passing to the ganglia ; and the post-ganglionic fibres, or those originating in the cells of the ganglia and passing to the periphery. Following the histological observations of Haskell,- previously quoted, and the physiological studies of Langleyj3 an outline of the relations of the sym- pathetic cells, based on the arrangement found in the cat, is briefly as follows: Pre-ganglionic fibre — i.e., those growing out of cell-bodies located in the 1 GaskeU : Journal of Physiology, 1885, vol. vii. ; von Kolliker, " Ueber die feinere Anatomie and die physiologische Bedeutung des sympathischen Nervensystems," Verhandlunf/en Gesell- 8chaft deulscher Naturforscher und Aertze, 194, Allgemeiner Theil, 1894. 2 Loc. cit. 3 Langley : " A Short Account of the Sympathetic System," Physiological Congress, Berne, 1895. Fig. 93.— Schema of the neurones forming the sympathetic nervous system (Huber: Journal of Com- paraif . L897, vol. vii.i. A solid black line designates the axone from an efferent neurone, with its cell-body in the ventral horn of the cord, and the terminal brush ending in a striated muscle (m.n.). A black line crossed by short dashes designates the axone from an afferent neurone, the cell-body of which is in the spinal ganglion, and the peripheral axone of which terminates in the epidermis or some special sense-organ (g.n.). An interrupted black Km indicates an axone of similar origin to the one just described, but distributed with the fibres of the sympathetic system (s.s/.). At the periphery it terminates in a free ending (s.s.f.H)) or in a Pacinian corpuscle (s.s/.(2)). A blue line shows a preganglionic fibre (of Langley), the cell-body of the neurone being located in the lateral horn of the cord. The axone leaves the cord by the ventral root (as a fibre of very small calibre), passes in the white ramus | W.R.), and terminates by a pericellular basket about the body of a sympa- thetic neurone (drawn in red). The various places where such an axone may terminate are indicated as follows: o. axone passing through the chain-ganglion (I.C.6.) to terminate within the next higher chain-ganglion ; b, axone passing as does (a), but terminating in the next lower chain-ganglion {II.C.O.) ; c, two axones ending in a gan- glion of this same segment (I. CO.); d, axone passing through the chain-ganglion of the segment and ending in a prevertebral ganglion (Pr.v.G.) : e, axone passing through both a chain-ganglion and a pre- vertebral ganglion to end in a peripheral ganglion I Periph.6.) ; /, axone which gives off a collateral branch to one ganglion {J.CO.) and passes on to terminate in a more distal ganglion (Pr.v.G.). Fibres arranged like (/) probably account for some of the reflexes obtained from sympathetic ganglia ; g and h. axones representing fibres which regularly pass to any given ganglion from the ganglia above and below it. The sympathetic neurones are drawn in red, and about their cell-bodies terminal baskets of other axones (always in blue) are shown. They enter the mixed nerve by the gray ramus (G.R.). m, the axones of the sympathetic neurones, terminate: i, in the muscular coats of the blood-vessels (vaso-motor endings); j, in the muscular coals of the viscera (viscero-motor endings), and in heart-muscle (not specially shown in the figure) ; k, in glands (secretory fibres); I, in other sympathetic ganglia (a doubtful form of termi- nation). The figure further shows two " afferent " sympathetic neurones (Dogiel), in dotted red ; o, arising in a peripheral ganglion [Periph.0.) and terminating in the prevertebral ganglion [Pr.v.O.) : p, arising in the chain ganglion (I.C.G.) and passing to the spinal ganglion, to terminate about Dogiel's spinal ganglion- cell of " type two," q (represented in solid back); g, spinal ganglion-cell (Dogiel's " type-two "), the ter- minals of W Inch form baskets about the bodies of the ordinary spinal ganglion-cells. CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. 219 cord — arise from the first thoracic to the fourth or fifth lumbar, and from these segments only (Gaskell). The fibres are medullated. Langley's experiments indicate that no sympathetic cell sends a branch to any other sympathetic cell, but other observers do not admit his results as conclusive. It has been shown that the pre-ganglionic fibres are interrupted in the ganglia. The post-ganglionic fibres are in part medullated, though sometimes medullation occurs only at intervals, but in the main they are gray or unmedullated. The cerebro-spinal axones end in the ganglia in such a manner that the branches of the pre-ganglionic axone are distributed to a number of the ganglion cell-bodies, and these cells in turn send their axones either directly to the peripheral structures controlled by the sympathetic elements or to the plexuses such as are to be found in the intestine and about the blood-vessels. The same pre-ganglionic fibre may have connections with several cells in one ganglion, or, by means of collaterals, connect with one or more cells in a series of ganglia (Langlev). Manner of Diffusion. — It has been found that while the cells in a sympa- thetic ganglion are so arranged that one pre-ganglionic fibre may be in connection with a group of cells, and thus the impulses which pass out of the ganglion be more numerous than those which entered it, yet the several groups of cells within the ganglion are not connected. In the peripheral plexuses there appears to be a different arrangement.1 It has been observed upon stimulation of the branches of the coeliac plexus in the dog that the several branches, though unlike in size, bring about nearly the same quantitative reaction in the constriction of the veins, from which we infer that though entering the peripheral plexus by different channels, the impulses find their way to the same elements at the end, owing to a multi- plicity of pathways within the plexus.2 Experiments with strychnin on the more proximal sympathetic ganglia do not show any increased diffusibility following the application of the drug ; but, on the other hand, Langley and Dickinson 3 have shown that nicotin applied to the superior cervical sympathetic ganglion of the eat produces a condition whereby electrical stimulation below the ganglion, which in the normal animal is followed by dilatation of the pupil, is without effect. Since the application of the drug to the nerve-fibres on either side of the ganglion is ineffective, when, at the same time, the application to the ganglion itself is effective, it is inferred that the drug acts by altering some peculiar relation existing within the ganglion, and the relation which is assumed to be thus modified is that between the fibres terminating in the ganglion and the cells which tiny there control. The passage of the efferent impulses through other sympathetic ganglia is likewise blocked by nicotin. Evidence for Continuous Outgoing- Impulses. — Under normal condi- tions striped and unstriped muscular tissues are always in a state of slight 'Berkeley: Anatomixrhcr Anzeitjrr, IS',1'2. 2 Mall: Arch irf. Anatomie u. Physiologic, 1892 3 Proceedings of the ltoyul Socirli/, 1889, vol. xlvi. 220 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. contraction or tonus. When the nerves controlling any such sot of muscles are cut, or their central connections injured, the muscles at first relax. If a frog be hung up vertically alter removing the brain, the cord remain- ing intact, it is found that the legs are slightly flexed at the hip and knee. If now the sciatic nerve be cut upon one side, the leg on the side of the section hangs straighter, indicating that the muscles have relaxed a little as the result of the section of the nerve; if, in the same animal, the smaller arteries in the web of the foot be examined both before and after the section, it is found that after the section they have increased in diameter. Con- versely, artificial stimulation of the peripheral stump causes a contraction of the vessels, but it is not possible in so rough a way to imitate the tonic contraction of the skeletal muscles. It is inferred from these experiments that normally there pass from the central system along some of the nerve-fibres impulses which tend to keep the muscles iu a state of slight contraction. Destruction of the entire cord abolishes all outgoing impulses, and produces a complete relaxation of these muscles. Though the intensity of these outgoing impulses is normally always small, yet it is subject to significant variations. The difference between the tone of the muscles of an athlete in prime condition and those of a patient recover- ing from a prolonged and exhausting illness is easily recognized, and this difference is in a large measure due to the difference in the intensity of the impulses passing out of the cord. Among the insane, too, the variations in this tonic condition follow in a marked way the nutritive changes in the central system, and both facial and bodily expression have a value as an index of the strength and variability of those impulses on which the tone of the skeletal muscles depends. Indeed, so wide in the insane is the variation thus brought about that when the expressions of an individual at one time in a phase of mental exaltation, and at another in that of mental depression, arc compared, it appears hardly possible that they can be those of the same person. This continuous outflow of impulses from the central system is indicated also by the continuous changes within the glands, and the variations in these metabolic processes according to the activities of the central system. Rigor Mortis. — Even in the very act of dying the influence of these impulses can be again traced. The death of the central nerve-tissues being expressed as a chemical change, causes impulses to pass down the efferent nerves, and these impulses modify those chemical changes which, in the muscles of a frog's leg, for example, lead to rigor mortis. It thus happens that a frog suddenly killed and then left until the onset of rigor, will under ordinary circumstances show rigor at about the same time in both legs. If, however, the sciatic nerve on one side be cut immediately after the death of the animal, the beginning of rigor in that leg is much delayed, thus showing that the nervous connection is an important factor in modifying the time of this occurrence (Hermann). The Nervous Background. — We return now to the conditions which CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. 221 modify the spread of the impulses within the central system, when this sys- tem is represented by the spinal cord of a reflex frog. Admittedly, there is in the case chosen but a fraction of the central system. It has been shown that all incoming impulses tend to spread over a large part of the central system. In a reflex frog, therefore, the cord is cut off from the remote effect of im- pulses which normally enter the system by way of cells located in the portion removed. Moreover, in the complete nervous system the incoming impulses tend to be transmitted to the cephalic end, and in some measure give rise to impulses returning within the central system and affecting the efferent cells. In a fragment of the central system like the cord such impulses taken up by the central cells must pass so far as the axones are intact; but as these for the most part end at the level of the section, such impulses are lost, in the physi- ological sense, at that point. The fact, therefore, that the experiments with spinal reflexes are con- ducted on a portion of the central system has two important physiological consequences. In the first place, there are wanting incoming impulses, direct or indirect, from the portion removed ; on the other hand, through the sec- tion of the afferent axones, in their course within the central system, there is a direct diminution in the number of the pathways by which the impulses arriving at the cord may be there distributed. It is most probable that in the frog, at least, the reduction of the central mass does not so much dimin- ish the number of pathways by which the impulses may be immediately distributed by way of the afferent and central elements, as it diminishes the number of impulses which by way of the portion removed arrive at the efferent cells and modify their responsiveness. The modification of the responsive cells under more than one impulse is well illustrated by an experiment of Exner:1 A rabbit was so prepared that an electric stimulus could be applied to the cerebral cortex at a point the excitation of which caused contraction of certain muscles of the foot. ( )ne of these muscles was attached to a lever so that its contraction could be recorded, and a second electrode applied to the skin of the loot overlying the muscle. The discharging efferent cells in the cord were in this case subject to impulses from two directions, one from the cortex and one from the skin of the foot. With a current of given strength stimulation of the cortex alone caused a contraction of the muscle, and stimulation of the skin of the loot alone, a similar contraction. When both were stimulated simultaneously the extent of the contraction was greater than when either was stimulated alone If now the strength of the stimulus applied to the skin of the paw was so reduced that alone it was inefficient, then a stimulus from the cortex which produced a reaction as indicated by the firsl cortical stimulus in fig. 9 1 {A, a), put the efferenl cells in such a condition that the stimulus from the skin (A, h, Fig. 1)4), applied within 0.6 of a second, produced a second con- traction of the muscle, although alone the stimulus from the skin had proved inefficient. Here the first efficient stimulus from the cortex had rendered ' Exner: Archivfur die gesammte Physiologie, Bd. xxvii. ■2-2-2 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OE PHYSIOLOGY. the discharging cell for a short period of time more excitable. In the same figure the record shows that ii'a longer interval — here more than three seconds — be allowed to elapse, then the second stimulus from the skin remains ineffi- cient. A similar relation between the two incoming impulses is also found to hold when the stimulus from the skin is made to precede. The curve B, Fig. 94, shows the results when both stimuli are inefficient. In this the stimuli [l> and a) produce no effect when given several seconds apart, but when they occur within a short interval (/>' and a') — in this case 0.13 of a second — a contraction of the muscle follows. These various experiments, taken together, show in a beautiful way that in the cases chosen the two sets of impulses tend to reinforce each other, whether they are efficient or inefficient, and without regard to the order in which they come. This relation between the discharging cell and those by which it is stimulated can be illustrated in still another way. It was observed by Jendrassik ' that when a patient was being tested for the height of his knee- kick, ;i voluntary muscular contraction, or an extra sensory stimulus, occur- Moremi-iit of paw. Stimtdatioti of cortex. ifl! " b' " paw. /.'.-.'.'(' .':■■• seconds. rlHHrArlrHriru-\i Fig. 94.— To show the reinforcing influence of stimuli applied to the cerebral cortex and to the skin of the paw, on the movements of the paw of a rabbit (Exner). The arrows indicate the direction in which the curves are to be read. In curve .1 the cortical stimulus at a causes a movement of the paw. Dermal stimulus, within a second, at b causes a movement of the paw. Cortical stimulus at a' causes a movement of the paw. Dermal stimulus several seconds later at V is ineffective. In curve li dermal stimulus :it b is ineffective. The cortical stimulus at a several seconds later is also ineffective. The dermal stimulus at 1/ is ineffective, but if followed within 0.13 second by a cortical stimulus at a' a move- ment of the paw occurs. ring about the same time that the tendon was struck, had the effect of increasing the height of the kick. This relation was studied in detail by Bowditch and Warren,2 and they were able with great exactness to measure the interval between the contraction of the muscle used for reinforcement and the time at which the tendon was struck. The curve shown in Fig. 95 represents the results of these experiments. It indicates that, up to 0.4 of a second, the closer together these two stimuli occur the greater the reinforce- ment. At an interval of 0.4 of a second no effect is produced by the muscu- lar contraction. Increasing the interval only very slightly has, however, the effect of greatly diminishing the height of the knee-kick — i. e., decreasing the strength of the discharge of the efferent cells — and this effect is not lost until the interval is increased to 1.7 seconds, when the voluntary muscular contraction ceases to modify the response. A given efferent cell is thus modified in its discharge according to the several stimuli that act upon it. 1 Dentsches Archir fiir klinische Medizin, Bd. xxxiii. 1 Journal of Physiology, 1890, vol. xi. CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. 223 Effects of Afferent Impulses. — Studies on inactivity show that a certain amount of exercise in any given cell is necessary for its proper nutrition, and if the excitation fall below the point which causes this, the responsiveness of the cell is diminished. For example, a strychnized reflex frog on being dipped into a solution of cocaine loses in so large a measure its irritability that its responsiveness falls far below that of a normal frog.1 In this case the central system is deprived by the action of the cocaine of the impulses which even in the absence of any special form of irritation normally arrive from the skin, and the abolition of these impulses causes a diminution in central responsiveness. Effects which can thus be accom- plished in a few seconds by cutting off the afferent impulses from the skin may of course follow any slow diminution in these impulses, although all 40- 30- 20- 10- 0 10- 20- 30- \ Normal. O.I"0.2" 0.4" 0.7- 10" 1.7" Fig. 95.=Showing in millimeters the amount by which the "reinforced" knee-kick varied from the normal, the level of which is represented by the horizontal line at 0, "normal." The time intervals elapsing hetween the clinching of the hand 'which constituted the reinforcement) and the tap on the tendon are marked below. The reinforcement is greatest when the two events arc nearly simultaneous. At an interval of 0.4" it amounts to nothing; during the next 0.6" the height of the kick is actually diminished the longer the interval, after which the negative reinforcement tends to disappear; and when 1.7" is allowed to elapse the height of the kick ceases to be affected by the clinching of the hand (Bowditch and Warren). such slow changes arc much more likely to be accompanied by some sort of compensation whereby oilier afferent impulses in a measure take the place of those which have been suppressed, 'fhe loss of these impulses which rouse the cells to activity is usually a more important condition than direct nutri- tive change, and must for this reason always be kept in view. Inhibition. — Evidence is accumulating to show that all the active tissues of the body may be influenced through their nerves in two opposite ways. That is, stimulation may increase or diminish their activity." Thus the physiological processes in the glands, nerve-centres, or muscles can be so varied. In most cases, nerves which cause inhibition are, except in the central nervous system, distinct from those causing increased activity. The 1 Poulsson : Archiv fiir experimentelle Pathologie and Pharmakologie, L885, Bd. \xvi. 'Meltzer: "Inhibition," JVeto York Medical Journal, May, 1899. 224 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. chemical changes in the inhibited muscle of the heart arc peculiar and different from those occurring during excitation. For instance, Gaskell ' found that a 'positive variation of the muscle current occurred in the hearts of both the tortoise and crocodile upon stimulating the peripheral end of the vagus; while stimulation of the accelerator nerves caused the usual negative variation. Further, Gaskell pointed out that during the inhibition of the heart-muscle the anabolic processes were in excess, so that the cessation of inhibition was followed by an increase in the strength of the heart-beats. If we now turn to the observations bearing on inhibition in the central nervous system, there are to be found numerous experiments, of which the following is a type: Let one leg of a reflex frog be stimulated by pinching it, or by dipping the toe iu weak acid : a withdrawal of the stimulated leg will follow. Now repeat the experiment, at the same time pinching or other- wise stimulating the skin on the opposite leg. It will then lie found that either the latent period of the reacting leg is much prolonged or that the reaction fails completely. This is a very simple example of a type of inhibition which is continually occurring. The inhibitory effects are, however, not limited to the motor responses of the central system. It is an observation of the ancients that the greater obscures the leaser pain, and, in a general way, all strong sensations prevent the appreciation of the weaker ones, whether they be in the terms of the same or of a different sense. AYithin the central nervous system very remarkable examples of inhibitory phenomena have been investigated, chiefly by Sherrington.2 Boubnoff and Heidenhain3 were the first to record the observation that under certain conditions stimulation of the cerebral cortex might cause a relaxation of some extensor muscles of the limbs when these were in a state of tonic contraction. Sherrington was able to show that the stimulation of the cortical area for the flexor- of the arms also gave rise to impulses leaving the cortex and causing a (inhibition) relaxation of the antagonistic extensors. ( )n stimulating the cortical ana for the extensor muscles a corresponding relaxation of the flexors could be observed. Thus the cortical area for the contraction of a given group of muscles coincides with the area for the inhi- bition of the group antagonistic to it. Sherrington has also demonstrated the important role of this inhibitory process in mediating muscular co-ordination shown in movements of the eye. When all the muscles of the eye are para- lyzed, the eyeball held by the connective tissues about it looks straight ahead. Sherrington cut the nerves to all the muscles of the left eyeball (monkey) except the external rectus. ruder these conditions the eye, when ;it rest, looked toward the left. Stimulation of the cortical centers, which cause a conjugate deviation of both eyes to the right, was followed by a 1 Graskell : Journal of Physiology, vol. vii. '-' Sherrington : Ibid., vol. xvii. 3 Boubnotr und Heidenhain: Pfluger'a Arckiv, xxvi. CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. 225 movement of the operated eye toward the median plane (the right), and i<> the position in which it would be held by the clastic tissues alone. This could be explained only through a relaxation or inhibition of the external rectus muscle, as a consequence of the cortical stimulation. Further experi- ments support the explanation, and also show that the cells, the activity of which is thus inhibited, must lie below the cerebral cortex, for the inhibition follows when the fibre-bundles below the cortex arc directly stimulated, the cortex having been first removed. The general bearing of these results is of the greatest importance. As has been pointed out by Hughlings-Jackson,1 damage of any sort to a portion of the nervous system may, in the simplest case, decrease the activity of the group of neurones controlled by the damaged part by cutting off the stimulat- ing impulses from them ; or, on the other hand — and this is often overlooked — it may permit them to become abnormally active by the stoppage of some impulses exerting an inhibitory control. Further, whether impulses from a given set of cells shall prove stimulating or inhibitory depends on the other impulses affecting the receiving cell group, and on the time relations between these several sets. This consideration serves to indicate the complex rela- tions which may underlie the manifestations of disease in the central nervous system. As to the mechanism for these inhibitory reactions, it can be safely said that for the most part the effects are not dependent on the existence of a special class of inhibitory nerves, and the most we can think of structurally is a different but not necessarily constant dendritic pathway for the ccllu- lipetal impulses causing inhibition. C. Reactions Involving the Encephalon. On attempting to distinguish between a voluntary and a reflex act from the physiological standpoint we find the chief difference to be that the voluntary act is not predictable, because, according to the capabilities of the animal, it may be more variable in form than is the reflex response, and also because, instead of necessarily occurring within a short interval after the stimulus, as does the reflex, the voluntary response may be delayed even for years. Reflexes have been illustrated by the reactions from a portion of the spinal cord. It is to be remembered, however, that any of the sensory cranial or spinal nerves can serve as a pathway for the afferent impulses, and any of the groups of efferent cells situated in the ventral horns or their liomologues in the brain stem, can carry the efferent impulses needed. Further, it musi be remembered that it is these same afferent cells which always furnish the first set of impulses, and the efferent cells controlling the muscles and glands which furnish the last set of impulses in both reflex ami voluntary reactions. The processes then which distinguish the two forms of reactions must take place in the central cells. We turn, therefore, t<» the nervous connections <>f the encephalon with the cord, since it is by means of these connections that 1 Hughlings-JacksoD : Lancet, 1898, vol. i. Vol. II.— 15 226 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. the impulses travel to the many series of central cells which are concerned in the simplest voluntary responses. For the most complex voluntary reactions the entire central system is necessary, and especially the cortex of the cerebral hemispheres, while it has already been shown that the impulses which cause reflex actions can make their circuit in a very limited portion of the spinal cord. In the case of voluntary reactions the impulses take a longer pathway and involve a larger series of central nerve-elements, since from the point at which they enter the system they must pass to the cephalic end and back again to the efferent elements. At the same time, in a voluntary reaction, a greater number of impulses combine to modify the discharge from the efferent cells. In order that the encephalon may be included in the pathway of the im- pulses entering the cord, it is necessary that pathways formed by axones should, on the one hand, extend up to the encephalon, and, on the other, back from it to the cord. Fig. 96 indicates how the first part of this path is composed of the afferent elements of the dorsal spinal fig. 9G.-schema showing the smaller pathway nerve-roots. The long paths in the of the sensory impulses, on the lett side, s, S', dorsal funiculi of the cord are formed represent afferent spinal nerve-fibres ;C, an afferent ... , _ . _ cranial nerve-fibre. This fibre in each case termi- by the ascending branches of the af- nates near a central cell, the axone of which ferCnt axoneg an(J tl1(,S(, terminate, for crosses the middle line and ends m the oppo- ' site hemisphere. The interruption of the larger the most part, about the cell-bodies pathway in the thalamus is not indicated (van \ • \ r t,\ 1 • C j.1 1 1 , ,e] ,,„.,,',,.„, winch form the nuclei of the dorsal funiculi at the junction of the cord and bulb. From these nuclei a second series of axones passes out, decussates al ->nce, and then the axones pass forward in the medial lemniscus to rind a second ending in the ventral cell masses of the thalamus, or possibly to continue up to the cortex. From this point a third group of neurones, with their cell-bodies in the thalamus, send out their axones to the cerebral cortex. The cranial afferent nerves, which are not nerves of specific; sensations (i. e., the fifth, the vestibular portion of the eighth, the ninth, and tenth), probably have corresponding connections in the central system. The impulses which are brought in by the afferent fibres also pass, in a large measure, to cells in the dorsal horn of the spinal cord by way of the CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. 227 collaterals and the ascending branches of the afferent axones, which end before they reach the nuclei of the dorsal funiculi. The cells in the dorsal horns send their axones in large numbers across the cord to the lateral columns of the opposite side, to reach the thalamus through the medial lemniscus, and thence to the cortex. Of the many disputed points in this pathway, the most important relates to the interruption of the axones of the lemniscus in the ventral portion of the thalamus. The recent researches of Tschermak ' indicate that probably there are two groups of neurones concerned, one of which sends its axones without interruption to terminate in the cortex, while the axones from the other are interrupted at the level of the thalamus. The latter group is the larger and probably the more important for the general reactions of the central system. The pathways which are here sketched have been worked out mainly by the study of degenerations, in large part resulting from experimental lesions. When the dorsal roots are crushed or sectioned between the spinal gan- glion and the cord, the prolongation of the afferent fibre within the cord degenerates throughout its entire extent. The degeneration extends in the dorsal columns down the cord two or three centimeters from the level of the section, and also up the cord as far as the nuclei of the dorsal columns located at the commencement of the bulb. If the section is made near the caudal end, the degeneration may in consequence run through the entire length of the cord. Moreover, it occurs mainly on the side of the cord to which the sectioned nerves belong. Take, for example, the area of degeneration caused by the section in a dog of the dorsal roots on the left side between the sixth lumbar and second sacral nerves. The degeneration in the lower lumbar region is represented in Fig. 97, a, in the upper lumbar region in b, and in the thoracic region in c and d. The section c passes through the cervical enlargement. On passing cephalad the area of degeneration becomes smaller. This is interpreted to mean that all along, between the caudal and cephalic limits, fibres are given off from the main bundle to the intermediate levels of the cord. Here is evidence of an arrangement that is always t<> be kept in view. Though a number of fibres among those degenerating alter section of the dorsal roots may run the longer course to the bulb, the larger portion run a short or an intermediate course, and are, therefore, distributed at dif- ferent points between the termini. Injury to the dorsal roots at different levels shows, moreover, that the fibres from a given level which run the length of the dorsal columns do not mingle indiscriminately with those from other levels, but form a bundle; and as that bundle passes cephalad in the cord, it tends to lie nearer the middle line. Hence in the upper cervical cord, where the bundles from all levels are present, a cross-sect ion shows the bundles which entered lowest down to be located nearest the dorsal surface and the median septum. From these relations it is evident that comparatively few of the dorsal root-fibres run the entire length of the dorsal funiculi, since the majority 1 Tschermak: Archivfur Anatomii v/nd Physiologie, Anat. Abthl., 1898, S. 291-400. 228 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OE PHYSIOLOGY. terminate somewhere between this extreme limit and their point of en- trance. Since the fibres1 in the dorsal funiculi of the cord degenerate on destruc- tion of the dorsal roots, it is inferred that they must be morphologically con- tinuous with certain fibres in the roots, and, since the dorsal roots are afferent pathways, they too musl form pari of the afferent pathway in the cord. The continuation of the other paths for the afferent impulses must, how- ever, be formed by the axones of the central cells with which the dorsal root-fibres conned a- they terminate at the several levels of the cord. Fig. '.'7.— The sections are from five levels of the spinal cord of a dog. The dorsal roots on one side had heen sectioned in two groups: first, the twenty-eighth, twenty-seventh, and twenty-sixth spinal nerves; and second, the twenty-second and twentieth: a, shows a schematic picture, representing a cross-section of the spinal cord taken just below the level of the twenty-second spinal root. The black spot represents the principal bundle of degenerated fibres as it appears in the dorsal column. At this level the bundle is rather near the median septum, but if sections further cam lail were examined in series it would be found thai the bundle constantly approached the dorsal horn, and finally fused with it at the level where the injured nerves joined the cord. If, on the other hand, a section be taken from the level between the t\\ enty-second and the twentieth nerves— that is, after passing the level at which the second group of sectioned nerves joins the cord — there arc to be seen two bundles of degenerated tihres marked by black spots in the sections ''*, C, d). The last bundle to enter the cord, and the one lying nearer the dorsal horn, is, of course, formed by the degenerating fibres from the second group of roots. In the sec- tions c, d, e, taken respectively at the level of the eighteenth nerve, the middle of the thoracic cord and the cervical enlargement, it is seen that both degenerated bundles grow smaller: that they shift toward the median Beptum and approach one another; and. finally, that they completely fuse in the cervical region Degeneration after Hemisection of Cord. — Upon hemisection of the cord involving one lateral half, the ascending fibres which degenerate appear in the dorsal columns, in the dorso-lateral ascending tract, and in the ventro- lateral ascending tract. The number of degenerated fibres is large on the side of the lesion, hut on the opposite side there are also degenerated fibres in all these localities, although tiny are by no means s<> numerous. It is inferred that all the fibres which thus degenerate form path.- for the afferent impulses. The impulses which come in over a dorsal root on one side can, therefore, find their way cephalad either by the direci continuation- of the dorsal root- fibres running in the dorsal column mainly on the side of the lesion or through the interpolation of central cells, the axones of which appear degen- 1 The bundles of "endogenous fibres" not arising from spinal ganglion-cells are neglected here. CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. 229 crated in both lateral columns, but more numerously on the side of the lesion.1 The tracts which undergo secondary degeneration after this treatment include, therefore, those formed by the axones arising from central cells. These neurones have their cell-bodies arranged in columns running the length of the cord. In the neighborhood of these columns some of the dorsal root- fibres terminate. In the bulb we are familiar with such groups of cells, well marked as the "nuclei of the dorsal funiculi or columns," and the corresponding cells in the cord, though far less clearly segregated, are the homologues of those in the bulb. If this is granted, then the fibres which are outgrowths from these central cell-groups, whether in the cord or bull), are also homologous. Corroborative of what has been said on the subject of afferent pathways in the cord are the results of Pelizzi.2 He studied dogs, making use of the method of Marchi, whereby the nerve-sheaths of fibres beginning to degene- rate or the nutrition of which is disturbed give a characteristic reaction. He found, after hemisection of the cord, the same lesions that have been described above, with the addition that the changes could also be followed in sonic of the fibres of the ventral roots. More significant, however, is the fact that section of the lumbar and sacral dorsal roots, without direct injury to the cord, gave rise to modifications of the medullary sheaths, detectable by the method of Marchi, in all the localities just named. A distinction must be made at this point, Secondary degeneration in the central system means eventual destruction of the severed fibre. The method of Marchi shows a characteristic change in fibres entering upon this degenera- tion, but this method also shows changes in the sheaths of elements which are only physiologically connected with those about to undergo secondary degeneration, but which themselves are, as a rule, not ultimately destroyed. Under the usual conditions of experiment, complete degeneration is confined within the morphological limits of a single cell-element, but the physiological changes in the cells overstep this limit, as shown by Marches reaction. Physiological Observations on Afferent Pathways. — Making use of the fact that strong stimulation of the sensory fibres, such as those in the sciatic nerve, causes a rise in blood-pressure, Wbroschiloff3 sought to block the passage of the impulses causing this reaction by section of the cord in different ways in the upper lumbar region of the rabbit. It appears that in this animal the reaction was most diminished — that is, stimulation of the sciatic produced least rise in the blood-pressure — when the lateral columns of the cord had been cut through; and that the effect of section of the lateral column on the side opposite to that on which the stimulus was applied was greater than the following section of the column on the same side. These experiments form a very definite part of the evidence which directs our attention to the lateral columns of the cord as a principal afferent pathway. ' KohnatSLTam: Neurologischea Centralbla.lt, L900, S. 242. '■ Archives iialiennes de Biologie, ls'.i">, t. sxiv. 3 Beriehteder math.-phys. Clawed. /.-. Qenellnch. >l. Wwsen. ~u Leipzig, 1874. 230 IV AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. The physiological observations of Gotch and Horsier1 indicate that when in a monkey a dorsal root is stimulated electrically 80 per cent, of the impulses pass cephalad on the same side of the cord, while the remainder cross. Of the 20 per cent, that cross, some 15 per cent, pass up in the dorsal columns. This leaves only 5 per cent, of the impulses to pass up by the contra-lateral columns. These experiments, therefore, give less impor- tance to the lateral columns than was to be expected from the observations of Woroschiloff. The dorso-ventral median longitudinal section of the cord in the monkey (sixth lumbar segment) 2 shows an ascending degeneration in a small part of the dorsal area of the direct cerebellar tracts and of the ventro- lateral tracts, as well as in the columns of Goll. This would indicate that the section had cut fibres which crossed the middle line and ran cephalad in these Localities. Osawa8 found that when the cord in a dog was hemisected (in the upper lumbar or lower thoracic region) the animal showed for the most part no permanent disturbance of sensation or motion. If the cord was first hemisected on one side, and later on the other side, the second hemisection being made a short distance above or below the first, sen- sation and motion persisted behind the section, although they were somewhat damaged. After three hemisections, alternating at different levels, there still remained a trace of co-ordinated movement possible to the hind legs, although the sensibility of the parts could not be clearly demonstrated. The path thus marked out for some afferent impulses is certainly a tortuous one, and at present not readily to be explained. Tt must be remembered, however, that our information concerning the short pathways in the cord is very slight. Nerves of Common Sensation. — In order to analyze the afferent path- ways still further, we next inquire whether among the dorsal nerve-roots which p.-iss between the cord and periphery there are separate nerve-fibres for each of the modes of sensation represented by pressure, heat, cold, pain, and the muscle-sensations. The data available for determination of this question are not of the best, but are still of some value. The number of dorsal root nerve-fibres on both sides was estimated (in a woman twenty-six years of age) by Stilling to be approximately 500,000.4 Stilling's estimate for the ventral root fibres in the same individual was 300,000. The area of the .-kin in a man of (>2 kilograms 1 136 pound-), and twenty- six years of age, was found by Meeh to lie 1,900,000 square millimeters.8 From tin' study of the nerves going to the muscles of the dog, Sherring- 1 Croon ian Lectures: Philosophical Transaction* <n persons in ordinary health. The capability of a given stimulus to produce pain is therefore subject to wide variations according to the general condition of the subject.3 The same stimulus ha- different effects in a given individual according to several circumstances. Peripheral irritation, such as an inflam- matory process in the skin, greatly increase- the intensity of the pain caused by the stimulation of the nerves supplying the locality. Continued stimula- 1 Sherrington : Journal of Physiology, 1894 5, vol. xvii. 2"T'cl)cr das Verhiiltniss des Calibers der Nerven zur Haut und den Muskeln des Men- schen." [naug. Diss. (Russian), 1883; vide OerUrcHblail fur NervenheUkunde, 1883, Bd. vi. "Strong: Psychological Review, 1895, vol. ii. No. 4. 232 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. tion of the sensory nerves of the muscles and viscera has the same effect.1 Local anaesthetics, such as cocaine, may reduce the sensibility to zero, and the same follows the general anaesthesia produced by chloroform, ether, nitrous oxide, morphine, and similar drugs. Painful sensations are distinct and powerful only when the stimulus is applied to general sensory nerve-trunks — /'. <■., those mediating cutaneous, muscular, and visceral sensibility — while the nerves which mediate the special sensations of light, sound, taste, and smell do not give pain even on excessive stimulation. Limiting our observation, therefore, to the nerves of cutaneous sensibility, it is found in exceptional cases that the sensations of pressure, heat, and cold may all be present to a normal degree, and yet increasing the stimulus he without effect in causing any painful sensations whatever. This would represent a condition of complete analgesia. Moreover, the capacity of the >kin to cause abnormal painful sensations upon the adequate stimulation of each of these groups of nerves may he associated (in lesions of the central system) with any one group alone, the abnormal pain-sensations thus pro- duced being either excessive or deficient. We advance the hypothesis, therefore, that each of these three sensations, if pushed to excess, is usually accompanied by pain of gradually increasing intensity. Therefore it is most probable that these nerves when slightly stimulated mediate their proper sensations, but when this stimulus is pushed to excess they can give rise to pain also, and that in the last instance this sensation of pain may prove exclusive of any other. H this view is correct, it appears improbable that special pain-nerves exist. As various experiments show, increasing either the strength of the periph- eral stimulus, the number of fibres to which it is applied, or the irritability of the terminals of the fibres, will assist in arousing painful sensations. In the last analysis the physiological condition for pain is excessive stimulation, which by all analogy must mean excessive discharge within the central system. The changes following this discharge into the central system are not such as lead to co-ordinated muscular responses, but to convulsive reac- tions of a very irregular character. Where this process takes place in the central system we do not know. As to normal analgesia, it must be looked upon as dependent on a condition in which excessive stimulation cannot be produced ; and we find this condition normally present only in the case of the nerves of special sense. Since in the pathological conditions one sort of sensibility may lie lost while the others remain, it has been inferred that there are separate fibres for the conveyance of each sort of sensation. This idea was expressed in the law of the specific energies of nerves as formulated by Joannes Muller, who pointed out that in many cases the same nerve might be stimulated in any way — mechanically, electrically, or chemically, as well as in the normal physi- ological ma nner; and that in all cases the mode of the response was the same — a sensation of light or taste or contact, as the case might be. Hence it was 1 Gad mill Goldscheider : Zeitschrift fiir klinische Me'dicin, Bd. xx. CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. 233 argued that the mode of the sensation was independent of the kind of stimu- lus, but dependent on the nature of the central cells among which the afferent fibres terminated. It will be seen, however, that this argument does not touch the character of the nerve-impulses in any two sets of nerves, and we have no observations by which to decide whether the nerve-impulses passing along the optic nerve-fibres arc, for example, similar or dissimilar to those which pass along the auditory fibres. If the nerve-impulses are always all alike, there seems no escape from the inference that separate nerve-fibres convey the impulses destined to give rise to different sensations. At the same time, it is just possible that the nature of the impulses and of the resultant sensation is, in the nerves of cutaneous sensibility, determined by the form of the peripheral stimulus, and that, as a consequence, different branches of the same nerve-fibres may be conceived of as susceptible to different forms of stimulation, and thus the two different sensations follow from the partial stimulation of the same nerve-fibres. The second possibility, that the nerve impulse has different characters in different afferent nerves, and further may be modified by the nature of the normal stimulus (pressure or temperature), is not to be too readily rejected, as Hering at least argues in favor of such a view'.1 Pathway of Impulses in the Spinal Cord. — The question arises bow these impulses are distributed among the afferent tracts which are recognized in the cord, and whether these tracts form special paths for the impulses that rouse the several sensations of pressure, temperature (heat and cold), and pain. Since it is necessary to know the sensations of the subject, this prob- lem can be, in some ways, best studied in man. Here, owing to wounds or disease, it may so happen that some of these sensations are lost or greatly diminished, and it is to be determined whether this loss is constantly associ- ated with the interruption of definite tracts. Unfortunately, however, the material for such a study is very meagre. In man the typical group of symptoms following hemisection of the spinal cord above the lumbar region has long been known as Brown-Sequard's paralysis. The clinical observations on cases suffering from such a lesion have been recently summarized by Oppenheim 2 as follows : 1. A paralysis of the homo-lateral muscles. In the ease of the leg. the effects are most intense and persistent in the flexors of the thigh and shank, and the extensors of the foot. 2. When the two sides of the body are contrasted, there appears to be a homo-lateral hyperesthesia, accompanied by contra-lateral anaesthesia. 3. As to the several forms of sensation, the following may be stated : («) The muscle sensations (Bathyasthesia, Oppenheim): the defect is never contra-lateral ; sometimes, however, it is bilateral, but in mosl cases is homo-lateral. (b) The contact sensations are very often not affected at all — sometimes 'Hering: "Zur Theorie der Nerventhatigkeit," Akademischer Vortrag, Leipzig, 1S99. 'Oppenheim: ArcMvfur Physiologic, Physiol. Al.tlil., Suppl. Bd., 1 licit. .Inly. 1899. 234 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. slightly, and in the latter case the hyperesthesia may appear on either or both sides. (c) The prominent and almost constant sensory symptom is the contra- lateral loss of the sensation lor pain and temperature. On the basis of a case1 in which the lateral columns of the cord and the gray matter of both horns on the same side were the seat of damage, and in which there was a total loss of pain on the opposite side of the body without impairment of tactile sensibility, it may be inferred that the pain-impulses cross soon after entering the cord, and pass cephalad by some path lying within the damaged area. A second case2 is recorded in which a stab-wound divided all of one-half of the cord pins the dorsal column of the other half. There was here a loss of sensibility to pain on the side opposite the lesion, together with the loss of tactile sensibility on both sides, pointing, therefore, to the dorsal columns as the paths for the tactile impulses. The experiments on the lower animals contradict this conclusion. The observations of Turner3 on monkeys, in which hemisection of the cord had been made in the lumbar and thoracic regions indicate that all sen- sory impulses cross immediately after entering the cord, yet section in the cervical region showed that the impulses roused by touching the skin pass in part on the same side of the cord as the section, the other sensory impulses being, however, completely crossed. On the other hand, from his work on hemisection of the thoracic cord of the monkey at different levels/ Mott found the disturbance of sensibility of all forms mainly on the side of the section. Hemisection of the Cord. — From experiments on monkeys and a few cats Schafer reports the following physiological changes after hemisection of the spinal cord in animals : " In the first few days complete motor paralysis of all part- supplied with nerves below the section. The limb or limbs on the paralyzed side swollen and warm (vasomotor paralysis) and lessened out- flow of lymph and the skin dry (diminution of sweat). Knee-jerk exag- gerated. Sensation not lost on the same side as the lesion, but at first appears dulled. (There is a difficulty in arriving at a clear decision on account of the motor paralysis rendering the animal unable to move the limb.) After a few days, unmistakable signs of feeling and localizing even a slight touch, and this long before the motor paralysis has passed oil'. The animals gener- ally disregard a clamp-clip on the skin of the paralyzed limb, but not always ; this phenomenon usually lasts until the return of movement in the muscles of the limb.'1 I have seen no signs of paralysis cither motor or sensory on 'Gowers: Clinical Society's Transactions, 1878, vol. xi. 2 Muller : /<"' itri'n/i -in- jiniliiiliii/ischi Aii'iiniiiii imil I'/niswIogie dea Riickenmarkes, Leipzig, 1871. 3 Brain, 1891. ' Mott : Journal of Physiology, 1891, vol. xvii. 5 It will be seen that my observations on this point agree generally with those of Mott (Phil. Trims. B. 1892), although my conclusions are somewhat different. Mott, in my opinion, lays too much stress on the results of the clip test (Schafer). CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. 235 the side opposite to the hemisection in any case in which this has been strictly confined to the one half of the cord.1 Sometimes the adjacent posterior column of the other half is injured, and in that event there is impairment of sensation for a time on both sides below the lesion. The motor paralysis, at first complete, becomes gradually incomplete, and finally is difficult or impos- sible to determine. But purely voluntary movements are not recovered or but very imperfectly, although all the ordinary associated movements of the limb are recovered. After about three or four weeks it is difficult to detect any sort of paralysis, but the limb which has been paralyzed is thinner than the other." If the hemisection is made above the level of the eighth cervical nerve, the pupil on the same side is relatively contracted and remains so. The dilator fibres and the pilomotor fibres in the cervical sympathetic do not degenerate, but remain excitable. The pupil reacts to light and shade in spite of its being persistently smaller than the other. Excitation of the motor cortex of the opposite cerebral hemisphere produces, as a rule, no move- ments in the limbs which have been paralyzed, even if the associated move- ments have long returned. As will be seen from the foregoing paragraphs bearing on the afferent pathways found in the spinal cord of man and the higher mammals, the evi- dence for the path of the cutaneous impulses is decidedly contradictory. In addition to the cutaneous impulses there are the sensory impulses from the viscera, muscles, and tendons, which find their path cephalad probably along the direct cerebellar tract as well as by the long pathways in the dorsal columns. After hemisection of the cord the "muscular" sensations are usually lost on the side of the section, and the observations of Tschcrmak, already mentioned, point to the long fibres in the dorsal funiculi as the path- way for the impulses from the muscles and joints. Indeed, the lack of good evidence for the conduction of any impulses — save those from the muscles and joints — by long tracts in the cord, has led Starr2 to suggest that the dermal impulses are transmitted by short path- ways through the cord. Since, then, the dorsal and lateral columns of the cord appear to contain the chief afferent paths for the sensory impulses, the next step in following the pathway is to find the terminations of these tracts, whether long or short. Of the latter nothing can be said, 'fhe long tracts in the dorsal columns are connected with the nuclei of those columns (nuclei of Goll and of Burdaeh) on the same side. The cells of these nuclei ^-\u\ their axones cephalad ; in part they decussate in the sensory crossing and contribute to the formation of the lemniscus, by way of which they pass either directly to the cerebral cortex about the central gyri, or reach this only after interruption in the 1 Ferrier and Turner ( Brain, 1891 I describe lose of sensibility in the opposite liind limb in the monkey. Brown-Sequard, as is well known, obtained 1 1 1 i » result in the rahbit. (See also Ferrier, Functions of the Brain, and Orooniam Lectures, 1890.) 'Starr: Transactions of the American Neurological Association, Twenty-third Meeting, 1897, p. 7. 236 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. thalamus. It will be remembered that these fibres of the dorsal columns are physiologically joined with the contra-lateral thalamus and hemisphere. In part, however, the axones from the dorsal nuclei enter the cerebellum by the inferior peduncle of the same side, and we shall refer to this when con- sidering the cerebellum. Cranial Nerves. — We shall next consider briefly the relations of the sev- eral atfcreiit cranial nerve.-, beginning with the vagus and working cephalad. Nervus Vagus\ Tenth Nerve). — The nucleus of termination for the afferent fibres of the tenth nerve (vagus) are shown in Fig. 91. The afferent fibres of this nerve are found to convey impulses which arise in the pharynx, oesoph- agus, stomach, liver, pancreas, spleen, larynx, bronchi, and lungs. Further, this nerve contributes afferent fibres to the nervus laryngeus superior. The location of the nucleus of termination (X. alse cinerse) falls within the area of the mi ml vital. Concerning the axones of the neurones forming the nucleus of termination, it can only he said that they are continued cephalad in the medial lemniscus and the fasciculus longitudinalis medialis. Nervus Glossopharyngeus [Ninth Nerve). — The ninth nerve (glossopharyn- geus, Fig. 91) i> represented in the hull) by the tractus solitarius, the fibres of which find their principal nucleus of termination in the cell-group lying ju-t to the medial side of the tract. The neurones of this nucleus send their axones cephalad by way of the medial lemniscus. The afferent fibres of N. glossopharyngeus mediate general sensations for the tonsils and pharynx, the tympanic cavity and Eustachian tube, while by way of the ramus lingualis ir innervates the taste-organs of the posterior part of the tongue and those in the pharynx. In addition to these fibres mediating the sense of taste, patho- logical evidence points to some additional fibres with the same function (not belonging to the ninth nerve) which reach the bulb by way of the fifth nerve and the nervus intermedins. The nuclei of termination for these three nerves are very close to one another in the bulb, and hence the innervation of a special -en-e-organ from three cranial nerves, which, in the first instance, seems anomalous, becomes more intelligible when it is recognized that the nuclei concerned are practically continuous. Nervus Intermedius. — In this connection the afferent fibres in the nervus intermedins (of Wrisberg) should be mentioned. These fibres arise from the cell-bodies of the ganglion geniculatum, enter the bulb between the super- ficial origin of the seventh and the vestibular root of the eighth nerve, and, running caudad along the dorso-medial tip of the ascending root of the fifth, finally terminate with the fibres of the glossopharyngeus in the cells of ter- mination found along the tractus solitarius. The longitudinal extension of these fibres of the nervus intermedius in the bulb closely matches that of the nucleu- of the eighth nerve, and at the periphery the fibres from the genicu- late ganglion are distributed, in part at least, with those of the seventh nerve.' Nervus Auditorius ( Eighth Nerve). — A. Cochlear Hoot. — The eighth nerve 1 Van Gehuchten : "Becherches sur la Terminaison centrale des Nerfs sensible peripheri- ques — le Nerf intermediary de Wrisberg." Le N&uraxe, Mars, 1900, t. i. fasc. 1. CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. 237 goes to the inner car. The cochlear portion of the inner car mediates sensa- tions of sound and is connected with the bulb by means of the aervus coch- leae; the cochlear branch of the eighth nerve. The cell-bodies of the aervus cochlea? are located in the spiral ganglion of the cochleae, which is homologous with the dorsal root ganglion of a spinal nerve. The ganglion cells are bipolar or diaxonic, one axonc passing toward the organ of Corti in the cochlea, and the other toward the bulb. On reaching the bulb, the nerve formed by the latter axones enters in a large measure the nucleus nervi cochlea? ventralis,1 and to a less extent the nucleus nervi cochlea? dorsalis. According to Held." some of the root-fibres entering the ventral nucleus may be continuous as far as the superior quad- rigemina, reaching that level by way of the trapezoid e um, the superior olive, the lateral lemniscus, and the colliculus inferior; to all of which gray masses, including the nucleus nervi cochlea? dorsalis, these axones may give collat- erals. Further, some fibres may terminate in any of the localities reached by the collaterals. Besides the direct continuations of the afferent axones by way of the ventral nucleus, each one of the localities mentioned above, includ- ing both the dorsal and ventral cochlear nuclei, contains cell-bodies forming, on the one hand, nuclei of termination, and on the other by their axones con- tinuing the auditory pathway even to the cerebral cortex (Held). A group of central cells with their bodies in the nucleus nervi cochlea? dorsalis send their axones across the floor of the fourth ventricle, forming the stria? acustica?. These axones in part decussate with the corresponding fibres — the crossing occurring in the raphe — and then either as direct or crossed fibres find their way cephalad by the same path (with some additions) as that described in connection with the ventral nucleus. B. Vestibular Root. — Quite separate from the cochlear is the vestibular division of the eighth nerve, and this separateness is a strong argument against the suggestion sometimes made that the portions of the labyrinth innervated by the vestibular nerve, may also mediate sensations of sound. The besf evidence shows the nerve to convey those impulses from the macula acustica utriculi and the crista' ampullares, which are largely utilized in the mainten- ance of the equilibrium and in arousing the sensations of the movement of the body as a whole. The peripheral neurones which give rise to the vestib- ular fibres have their cell-bodies collected in the vestibular ganglion. The peripheral axones of the ganglion-cells end among sensory epithelium of the parts just named, while the central axones, forming a larger rool than that associated with the cochlea, join the bulb at the caudal (■d^c of the puns, the vestibular root lying to the cephalic side of the cochlear root. Having entered the bulb, the axones divide, after the manner of dorsal root fibres, into an ascending and a descending branch, which find their nuclei of termina- tion in :i (1) the nucleus nervi vestibuli spinalis (the radix descendens); ('_') in 1 Barker: The Nervous System "nil its Constituent X* "nun*, 189'.*, pp. "ill 555. 2 Held : Arehivfur Physiologie, Aunt. Abth., Leipzig, L893. 3 Barker: '/'//< Nervous System mid Us Conxliiurnt \i-iirmim, 1899, p. <>_!7. >i .« Barker's The Nervous System, 1899. 3 Archiv fur Anatomie und Physiologie, INTO. Vol.. II.— 16 242 AN AMERICAN TENT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. apply the faradic current by means of fine but blunt electrodes, the ends of which are but two or three millimeters apart, to the exposed surface of the cerebral hemispheres, the pia being undisturbed. Rabbits, dogs, and monkeys have been the animals most commonly studied. If the current be slight, its application for one or more seconds causes a response in the shape of movements of muscles, which are thrown into co-ordi- nated contraction. The contraction continues for some time after the stim- ulus has been removed. When the stimulus is very strong, instead of a lim- ited and co-ordinated response, there may be a widespread contraction of Fig. 99.— Brain of the macaque monkey, showing the sensory and motor areas. In the sensory region the name of the sensation is over the locality most closely associated with the corresponding sense-organ; in the motor region the name of the part is written over the portion of the cortex which controls it. The upper figure gives a lateral view of the hemisphere, and the lower a dorsal view (Beevor and Horsley). many muscles, resembling an epileptic convulsion. This, however, occurs more commonly in the lower than in the higher mammals. On the other hand, the irritability of the cortex is easily reduced, so that it becomes irre- sponsive, ami often immediately after the first exposure of the brain there is a time during which no response can lie obtained. Deferring for a moment the evidence by which the sensory characters of the several areas have been established, and also the arrangements within the cortex by which any group of muscles can be made to respond to stimuli arriving at any sensory area, we shall follow out the distribution of those CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. 243 cortical cells which, on direct stimulation, cause contractions of the skeletal muscles. The results here presented were obtained from the electrical stimulation of the monkey's brain by Beevor and Horsley1 (see Figs. 99, 100). These experimenters explored the exposed surface of the hemisphere with the elec- trodes, moving them two millimeters at a time, and at each point noting the muscle-gronp first thrown into contraction. As the result of many observations on the monkey, it is possible to map out the cerebral cortex in the following way : The surface of the hemispheres is divided into regions (motor and sensory regions), which are the largest subdivisions. These are subdivided into areas for the muscle-groups belong- ing to different members of the body — arms, head, trunk, etc. — as well as those areas within which all the impulses from a given sense-organ reach the PoF. Fig. 100.— Mesial surface of the brain (monkey). The localization of motor functions is indicated along the shaded portion of the marginal gyrus. The location of the visual area is indicated at the tip of the occipital lobe, and the location of the olfactory area at the tip of the temporal (Ilorsley). cortex. The areas in turn are subdivided into centres, comprising the groups of cells, which, for example, control the smaller masses of muscle1 belonging to a given segment of a limb, or in the visual area constitute those cells espe- cially connected with one part of the retina. There is thus a motor region, the stimulation of which gives rise to the more evident bodily movements. Within this are several subdivisions, the stimulation of one of which is fol- lowed by movements of groups of muscle- — for instance, those controlling the arm — and within such an area in turn come the smaller centres, or those the stimulation of which is first followed by movements at one joint only. The physiological characters of these cortical motor centres have been determined by the following observations: If a vertical incision be carried around such a centre so as to isolate it from the other parts of the cortex, the characteristic reactions still follow the stimulation of it, indicating that the special etl'eet can lie produced by the 1 Beevor and Horsley: Philosophical Transactione of the Royal Society, 1SS8-90. 1244 AN AMERICA X TEXT- HOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. passage of impulses from the point of stimulation toward the infracortical structures. If, in addition, a cut be made below the cortex and parallel with its surface, then stimulation of the cortex above this section is ineffective, thus indicating that the impulses pass from the cortex directly into the substance of the hemisphere along certain nerve-tracts, which by this operation were sectioned. Further, if the bit of cortex thus separated from the underlying white substance be removed and the faradic current be applied to the white substance beneath, a reaction of the same type and involving the same mus- cles can be obtained, although it differs from that to be gotten from the cor- tex itself, in the first place by being less co-ordinated, in the second by con- tinuing only so long as the stimulus lasts, and in the third place by giving rise to less intense electrical changes connected with the passing impulse. By careful exploration the bundle of fibres which is thus picked out can be followed, as the brain substance is cut away, through the internal capsule and the cerebral peduncles. These facts taken together lead to the conclusion that when the cortex is stimulated the impulses concerned in producing the muscular contractions traverse cell-bodies at the point of stimulation, and are transmitted thence through the underlying fibres. AVe shall see later that this direct course probably does not represent the sole pathway for these impulses. Course of the Descending Impulses. — The course of the impulses is next inferred from the relation between the removal of different parts of the cortex and the consequent secondary degenerations throughout the length of the central nervous system. When the part of the cortex removed is taken from the motor area then the degeneration occurs in the internal capsule and in the callosum. The path of the fibres forming outgrowths of the cortical cells can be followed thence through the crusta and pyramids to the spinal cord. After removal of the motor region of one cerebral hemisphere the degen- eration is mainly in the internal capsule and crusta of the same side, though by way of fibres crossing in the callosum it may be traced to the other side also. At the decussation of the pyramids the fibres occupying the internal capsule of the same side as the lesion for the most part cross the middle line. The portion which remains uncrossed passes as the direct pyramidal tract of the ventral columns in man, while the crossed bundle, which is much the larger, lies in the dorsolateral field of the lateral column, forming the crossed pyramidal tract. Since the observations of Pitres1 in 1881-82 evidence has been accumulating to show that in man a lesion of the motor cortex of one cerebral hemisphere is followed by a degeneration of the crossed pyramidal tract on both sides of the cord. Of course, the degeneration in the hetero- lateral tract is much the larger of the two. That the fibres degenerating in the homolateral tract remain on the same side throughout their entire course is shown by the physiological experiments of Wcrtheimer and Lapage2 on 1 ProgreS medieale, Paris, 1882, x. 528. 1 Archives de Physiologic, 1897, No. 1, p. 168. CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. 245 dogs, and by the studies of Melius1 on secondary degenerations occurring in the cord after very limited lesions of the motor cortex of monkeys. The direct pyramidal tracts are well marked only in man. They usually disappear in the mid-thoracic region, having entered the gray substance by way of the ventral commissure, in which they undergo decussation. The crossed pyramidal tract shows the greatest diminution in area after passing caudad of the cervical and lumbar enlargements respectively, and hence it is inferred that the pyramidal fibres largely terminate at these levels of the cord. Fig. 101.— Schema of the projection-fibres within the brain (Starr) : lateral view of the internal cap- sule: A, tract from the frontal gyri to the pons nuclei, and so to the cerebellum ; />'. motor tract : I ', sen- sory tract for touch (separated from B for the sake of clearness in the schema) ; />, visual tract ; E, audi- tory tract; F, Q, II, superior, middle, and inferior cerebellar peduncles; J, fibres between the auditor; nucleus and the inferior quadrigeminal body ; K, motor decussation in the bulb; 17, fourth ventricle. The numerals refer to the cranial nerves. The sensory radiations kare seen to be massed toward the occipital end of the hemisphere. Sherrington has put forward the view that the pyramidal fibres recross in the cord, these recrossing fibres being derived in large part from a division of the pyramidal fibres into two branches, one of which may cross fco the opposite side of the cord, while the other continues its lirst course. Such dividing fibres he designates as"geminal fibres;" and the number of them is by no means small. The observations of Sherrington were made on monkeys (Macacus) aud 1 Proceedings of the Royal Society, London, 1891 and 1895. 246 IV AMERICAN TEXT- BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. dogs, and probably the arrangement of these fibres in man is similar. The observations are particularly significant as giving another anatomical basis for the control of the movements in both halves of the body from each cerebral hemisphere. The continuous degeneration, coupled with the histological evidence for the absence of intervening nerve-cells, indicates that the cell-bodies in the cortex have axones that extend all the way to the cell-groups of the spinal cord, even as tar as the sacral region. The usual picture of the final connec- tions of the pyramidal fibres shows the collaterals as coming into contact with the large cells in the ventral horns of the cord. On the ground of recent experiments on monkeys and cats, Schafer1 denies the existence of such ;i din zanoff: Gaz. da Hdpil., 1892. CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. 247 groups, because the number of pyramidal fibres is very much less than is the number of cells which they control. This discrepancy is in sonic measure relieved by the formation of "geminal" fibres already described. Moreover, the branching of the pyramidal fibres near their termination is very probable, and the most plausible view at present is that each pyramidal fibre by means of its collaterals controls, perhaps indirectly, a considerable number of cord cells, and probably the cells controlled by any one fibre form a more or less compact group. Mapping- of the Cortex. — Having sketched the relations of the pyramidal cells forming the motor region of the cerebral cortex to the parts lying below, we turn to study the arrangement, size, subdivisions, and comparative anatomy of this region, and then to examine the relation of it to the other parts of the cortex. The observations here quoted are those on the monkey only. On glancing at Fig. 99 it is evident, first, that the areas for the face and leg are widely separated from each other, that the arm-area lies between them, and that the area for the trunk, though less schematically placed, is located between that for the arm and leg. This arrangement is more typi- cally represented on the mesial (Fig. 100) than on the convex surface of the hemisphere, and in the former locality the serial order of the cortical areas corresponds with the order of the muscle-groups which they control. The Size of the Cortical Areas. — Evidently there is no direct relation between the extent of a cortical area and the mass of muscles which it con- trols. Certainly in man the mass of muscles in the leg is three times greater than that in the arm, and this latter many times greater than that of the face and head ; yet it is for the last area that the greatest cortical extent is found. Mass of muscle and extent of cortical area do not therefore iro together. When the movements effected by the muscles represented in these several areas are considered, we find that such movements become more complex and more accurate as we approach the head, and it therefore accords with the facts to consider the extent of the motor anas as correlated with the refinement of the movements which they control — a relation which may depend even more on the multiplication of the pathways bringing in impulses than on those which send them out. Subdivision of Areas. — The areas which have been described are further subdivided, the subdivisions in the arm-area being the clearest. Here it is found that the stimulation of the upper part of the arm-area gives rise to movements which start at the shoulder, while stimulation at the lower part of this area gives rise to movements first involving the lingers, ami especially the thumb. The centres from which these several reactions may lie obtained occupy, as Fig. 99 shows, narrow fields across the cortex in a fronto-occipital direction. Moreover, the centre for the most proximal joint of the arm is farthest removed from that for the most distal, while the intermediate joints are represented by their several centres lying in regular order between these two. A similar arrangement appears in the subdivisions of the cortex con- trolling the leg, and in the face-area as well. 248 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. Interpreting these facts in the terms of nerve-cells and their arrangement, it appears that in the shoulder-centre the axones of the cortical cells that discharge downward affect predomi- nantly the efferent cell-groups which in the spinal cord directly control the muscles of the shoulder, and that a similar arrangement obtains for the other centres in this region with the corresponding cell-groups in the cord. The stimulation of the different por- tions of the internal capsule where it is composed of bundles of fibres coin- ing from the motor region shows (ob- servations on orang-utang) that the fibres running to the several lower centres are there aggregated and arranged in the same order as the cortical centres from which they arise (see Fig. 102). Separateness of Areas and Cen- tres.— As we ascend in the mammalian series there is an increase in the per- fection with which cells forming the several centres are segregated, though the areas in the different orders tend to hold the same relative positions.1 Figs. 103, 104 give the localizations obtained in the rabbit's brain by stim- ulation (Mann). The various areas occupy a large proportion of the cortex, , and in some cases come very close to- Fig. 102.— Horizontal section of the human cere- J brum, showing the internal capsule on the left gethcr, SO that thev are not easily Sepa- side: /', frontal region: G, knee of the capsule ; , , . ' NC.NC, muriate nucleus; NL, lenticular nucleus; rated by experiment. 0, occipital lobe; TO, thalamus; X,X, lateral ven- Jn the lower monkeys (Macacvs trir-lc In the internal capsule the letters indicate the probable position of the bundles of tii.r.-s whir h Milieux) these cell-groups are SCgre- upon stimulation give rise to movements of the w^l g0 <),.,, those associated with the l>urts named or which convey special sets of in- © > _ i i » coming impulses: E, eyes ; ff.head; r.tongue; M, cervical portion Of the cord and lomi- mouth : L, shoulder; B.elboy ; D, digits; .A.abdo- ,i __ __„_i, .„..„„,, *■„ , ' ,. , ... . . mtr the arm-area are nmen more to- men ; P.hip; if, knee; U, toes; S, temporo-occip- a itai tract ; oc, fibres jo the occipital lobe ; op, optic gether and quite separate from those radiation (based on Ho rel . , ■, ..i ,1 1 i • ,1 associated with the lumbar region, the leg-area. In the orang-utang,- and to a greater extent in man, a further sepa- 1 Mann : Journal of Anatomy and Physiology, 1895, vol. xxx. 2 Beevor and Horsley : Proceedings of the Royal Society, London, 1890-91, vol. xlviii. CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. 249 Fir.. 103.— Rabbit's brain, dorsal view. The areas indicated are those the stimulation of which causes a movement of the parts named (Mann). Fig. 10-4. — Rabbit's brain, lateral view. The areas indicated are those the stimulation of which causes a movement of the parts named (Mann). Fig. 105.— Lateral view of the left hemisphere of an orang-utan^', showing the motor area about the cen- tral fissure (Beevor and Horsley). Kin. loo.— Lateral view of a left human hemisphere, showing the motor areas in man. The schema is based on the observations on the monkey, on pathological records (human), and on direct experiments on man. it is to be remembered that in the buman brain the excitable localities are surrounded bj rather extensive anas nol directly excitable (Dana). ration occurs, so that these centres come to be surrounded by parts of the cortex from which no response can be obtained upon direcl stimulation (see Fie;. 105). 250 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. By a few direct experiments and by many pathological observations some- thing is known of the motor centres in the human cerebral cortex. When K 1 1 ; . 107.— Mesial view of a human hemisphere, showing motor areas. Formed in the same way as Fig. 100. the results are plotted they give a distrib At the same time all such figures arc lari FlG. 108.— Frontal section of human cerebrum on the left side. The fibres forming the internal capsule ( ), the callosum ( ), and the anterior COmmisMirr i . - . — . — ) have been i mlic:i t <■_( tin- Royal Society, 1888, vol. xliii. 2 Sherrington : Journal of Physiology, 1 897—1898, vol. xxii. 252 AN AMERICAN TEXT- BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. between the olivary bodies. According to Spitzka,1 these bundles are absent in the case of the elephant and porpoise, :i condition which is correlated with the slight differentiation of the limbs, which are not modified either for fine movements or for tactile purposes. It has been pointed out, too, that removal of a hemisphere causes in the dog and most rodents a degeneration of other parts of the cord (dorsal columns) than those occupied by the pyramidal tracts in man.2 The fibres passing from the cortex to the efferent cell-groups in the cord do not, therefore, hold exactly the same position in various mammals. Size of Pyramidal Tracts. — It has been clearly shown that if the cross- sections of the r>'\\\> of the dog, monkey, and man be drawn of the same size, the pyramidal fibres being indicated, then the area of this bundle is propor- tionately greatest in man and least in the dog, the monkey being inter- mediate in this respect. The relations thus indicated are evident — namely, that the number of fibres controlling the cell-groups in man is the largest, and also is much larger than that in the lower animals. The relative areas of the pyramidal tract at corresponding levels, the ana of the entire cord being taken as 100 per cent., are given by v. Len- hossek3 for the following animals: Mouse 1.14 per cent. Guinea-pig 3.0 " Rabbit 5.3 " (at 7.76 " Man 11.87 « This relation is to be carefully noted, for with it is correlated the degree of the disturbances in the reactions of the entire nervous system following removal of parts of the cerebrum, the effect being slight when the cerebrum is connected with the cord by a small number of fibres, and serious when the connection is by many fibres, as in the case of man and the highest mammals. E. Localization in the Cerebral Cortex of the Cell-groups RECEIVING THE AFFERENT IMPULSES. Sensory Regions. — If an attempt is made to unify the construction of the entire cortex by bringing the motor and sensory areas under a common law, it must be based on the fact that the system of axones bringing impulses to the motor region forms part of the pathway for conducting the afferent im- pulse.- t'roin the skin and muscles back to some organ controlled by the efferent nerves. To Munk4 is due the credit of having from the first looked upon the responsive cortex as marked off into areas within which certain groups of these fibres terminate, so that apart from the sensory anas named from the special senses, he calls the area which controls the skeletal muscles the "Fuhlsphare" or body-sense area, on the assumption that in it end the : Journal of Gomparatiw Medicine and Surgery, 1886, vol. vii. a von Lenhossek : Anatomi*cher Anzeiyer, 1889. 3 THefeiner Bait des Nervensystemi im l/ichte neuester Forschungen, Basel, 1893. * Ueber die Functionen der Qro&hirnrinde, 1881. CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. 253 fibres bringing in impulses which arise through the stimulation of the skin and muscles. It has been suggested, to be sure, that separate localities form the seat for the dermal and muscular sensations. Ferrier indicated the limbic lobe, especially the hippocampal gyrus, while Horsley and Schafer argued for the gyrus fornicatus. At present, the weight of evidence is in favor of the location of the centres for dermal and muscular sensations in the central gyri, a part just caudad to and a part overlapping the area stimulation of which causes the muscles of the trunk and limbs to contract. Both in monkeys and in man defects in sensation are not permanent after limited lesions of the cortex, but, as suggested by Mott, the wide distribution of the incoming impulses would explain this result. Thus the entire portion of the cortex to which a definite function can be assigned must be looked upon as containing fibres which bring impulses into it, and cell-bodies which by their discharge send impulses to other divisions of the central system as well as to other parts of the cortex itself. All parts of the cortex having assigned functions give rise on stimulation to move- ments ; but in the case of the sensory areas, so called, they involve the con- tractions of only those muscles controlling the external sense organ, as the eyeball, external ear, tongue, and nostrils.1 Though physiologically impor- tant, and in the case of the eye reaching a high degree of refinement, they are quantitatively very insignificant when compared with the responses to be obtained from stimulating the "motor region," from which contractions of the larger skeletal muscles are obtained. Hence the usual terms " sensory " and " motor " do not completely characterize the corresponding regions, though they emphasize their most striking features. Determination of the Sensory Areas. — Using as a guide the appearance of the medullary sheaths upon the projection-fibres of the cerebral cortex of man during the last months of foetal life and shortly alter birth, Flechsig2 lias been able to outline the sensory areas in the cortex with great clearness. The illustrations from Flechsig (Figs. 109, 110) show the parts of the brain where the projection-fibres can be determined at a time when these fibres constitute all or almost all the medullated fibres connecting the cortex with the stem and basal ganglia. By thus marking out in color on the developing cortex the portions concerned, there are seen to be four main areas: First, the area connected with the olfactory trad (olfactory area), involving the uncinate gyrus, the gyrus hippocampi, and the part of the gyrus fornicatus nearest the callosum. Second, the area connected with the optic radiation (visual area), where tin; fibres in question are mosi abundant about the calcarine fissure. They appear, however, all through the cuneus and extend to the cortex which surrounds it on the ventral and lateral aspects of the occipital lobe. Third, we have (auditory area) the portion of the cortex which covers the transverse gyri in the Sylvian fissure and the first temporal gyrus where the former join it. This area is occupied by the project ion- 1 Ferrier : Functions of the Brain, 1876. 'Flechsig: Gehim und Seele, Leipzig, 1896, 254 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. fibres which convey the impulses arriving over the auditory nerve (the coch- lear branch of* the eighth). Finally the fourth area is seen (area for "body- Fig. 109.— The colored portion about the cuneus, especially that more deeply colored about the cal- carine fissure, shows the visual area as seen from the mesial surface. The portion comprising the deeply colored tipof the hippocampal gyrus, the dorsal portion of the hippocampal gyrus, and the edge of the gyrus fornicatus through its entire extent, marks the olfactory area. The remaining portion, occupying the paracentral gyrus and the mesial aspect of the first frontal gyrus, marks the mesial extension of the body-sense area. The uncolored portions of the cortex form the association centres of Flechsig. F, pes ; HS, crura; Z, pineal body ; 1, corpus albicans ; 2, chiasma ; S, anterior commissure ; 4, quadrigemina ; 5, callosum ; 6, fornix; 7, septum lucidum (from Flechsig). Fig. no.— The colored portion at the tip of the occipital lobe represents the postero-lateral extension ofthe visual area. 'I he colored portion about the central fissure and the neighboring parts of the frontal lobe represents the lateral extension ofthe body-sense area. The colored portion about the caudal end of the first temporal gyrus, and extending over the transverse gyri within the sylvian fissure, represents the auditory area. In all three areas the portions most deeply colored represent theareas where the projec- tion-fibres are most abundant. The uncolored portions of the cortes form the association centres of Flechsig i from Flechsig). sense"), which is most richly supplied with projection-fibres about the central fissure, in the two central gyri — but also extends forward on the lateral sur- CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. 255 face to include part of all the frontal gyri, while on the mesial surface the cortex concerned extends forward from the precuneus over more than half of the mesial surface. In this last area are delivered the afferent impulses from the skin, muscles, joints, viscera, and the lining walls of the alimentary tract. Flechsig points out that the projection-fibres, according to which these areas have been denned, are composed of axones bringing impulses to the cortex, and hence are sensory, in the usual terminology. The areas thus bounded are found to coincide with the areas which (in animals) respond to direct stimulation by the contraction of definite groups of muscles. The earlier determinations of the sensory areas in man were made from the study of brains modified by destructive lesions or congenital defects. The cortical centre for smell, inferred from comparative anatomy and physiology to be closely connected with the hippocampal and fornicate gyri and the uncus, has been similarly located in man on the basis of pathological observations ; but the evidence lacks precision. Concerning the location of taste sensations, very little is known. Both of these senses, it must be remembered, are insignificant in man, and hence their central connections are not easily studied. On the other hand, the cortical areas for hearing and sight have been located with much more precision and certainty. Damage to the posterior third of the first temporal gyrus and the asso- ciated gyri transversi causes in man deafness in the opposite ear, and con- cordantly conditions of the ear which early in life lead to deafness and deaf- mutism are accompanied by a lack of development in these gyri.1 Destruc- tion of this area on one side causes slight deafness mainly in the opposite ear, while complete deafness follows a cortical lesion only when it is double. In the case of the visual areas in man there is the same sort of evidence, but somewhat more exact. The destruction of the area represented by the cuneus and the surrounding cortex (Figs. 109 and 110) always injures vision, the maximum disturbance following injury to the cortex of tin; calcarine fissure. Conversely, the failure of the eyes to grow, arrests the development of this portion of the hemisphere. Hemianopsia. — Tt is found, moreover, that injury to the visual area in one hemisphere usually produces a hemianopsia or partial defect of vision in both retina'. The homonymous halves are affected on the same side as the lesion and the dividing line is usually vertical. The clinical picture corre- sponds to a semi-decussation of the optic tract and the representation of the homonymous halves of each retina in both hemispheres. At the same time the relation is much more complicated than at firsl sight appears, for the point of most acute vision is often unaffected in such cases. This peculiarity depends apparently on the (act that there is a binocular centre for macular vision in the cortex lining the sides ami bottom of the posterior portion of the calcarine fissure.2 1 Donaldson: American Journal <>/" Psychology, 1891- 2. vol. iv. 2 Laqueur and Schmidt : Virchotfs Archiv, 1899, Bd. 158, Heft 3, S. 467. 256 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. In the case of neither vision nor hearing do we find in man any subcortical cell-groups capable of acting as centres ; that is, after the destruction of the appropriate cortical region the corresponding sensations and reactions to the stimuli which arouse these sensations are completely and permanently lost. From these facts, therefore, it appears that the impulses which give rise to visual and auditory sensations are delivered in certain parts of the cerebral cortex, and unless they arrive there the appropriate sensations are wanting. Association Fibres and Association Centres. — Common experience shows us that we can voluntarily contract any group of muscles in response to any form of stimulus — dermal, gustatory, olfactory, auditory, or visual. When, therefore, the hand is extended in response to a visual stimulus, the Fig. 111.— Lateral view of a human hemisphere, showing the bundles of association-fibres (Starr): A, A, between adjacent gyri; Ii, between frontal and occipital areas; C, between frontal and temporal areas, cingulum ; D, between frontal and temporal areas, fasciculus uncinatus; E, between occipital and temporal areas, fasciculus longitudinalis inferior ; C, N, caudate nucleus; 0, T, optic thalamus. nerve-impulses pass first to the visual area, and then in an indirect manner arouse the cortical cells controlling the muscles of the hand. This connec- tion of the two areas is accomplished through the so-called association-fibres of the cortex. These fibres are formally defined as those which put into connection different parts of one lateral half of any subdivision of the central system (see Fig. 111). The bundles which are thus shown in the cerebral hemisphere must be looked upon as typical of the arrangement throughout the entire cortex, and, further, the arrangement in the cortex is typical of that in other parts of the central system. Anatomy would suggest, and pathology would bear out the suggestion, that it is by these tracts that the impulses travel from one area to another. CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. 257 The term "association centres" is applied by Flechsig1 to those portions of the cerebral cortex that lie between the sensory centres which he has been able to demonstrate. The functions of the association centres are first to furnish pathways, more or less intricate, between the several centres, and second, to retain as memories previous sense impressions, so that in acting they also modify the impulses sent into them, and by these modifications shade and adjust to an almost infinite degree the form of the final response. On looking at Figs. 109, 110, we note two well-defined areas: (1) that occupying the frontal lobe and forming the great anterior association centre, and (2) the area in the parietotemporal region which forms a second, the posterior association centre. The third, the middle association centre coin- cides with the Island of Reil, and is much less in evidence. On comparison it will be seen that these regions correspond to what have been called the " latent areas " of the cortex, because no evident response follows the direct stimulation of them. When we compare the extent of these association centres in man with that in other mammals, even the apes, we find the human brain characterized by the high development of these portions. Thus Flechsig feels justified in speaking of these association centres as the "organs of thought," and in pointing out how by means of them the incom- ing sense impressions are made to interact on one another, and in combination with the memory images which are thus aroused give rise to new ideas. The association processes carried on by these several centres, are modified by their location, so that the several centres have different and distinct values. With the disturbance of these association centres are correlated the several sorts of mental defects which have been gathered under the term aphasia. Aphasia. — The development of the ideas bearing on this subject has been slow. After the publication of the great work of Gall and Spurzheim (1810— 19) on the brain, some pathologists (Bouillaud, 1825; Dax, 1836), especially in France, were in seach of evidence touching the doctrine of the localization of function. At the same time the subject of phrenology , as put forward by Gall and Spurzheim, was not in good repute, and anything which looked that way, even in a slight degree, was generally scouted. Broca, however, published (1861) the important observation that when the most ventral or the third frontal convolution in the left hemisphere (often designated Broca's eon- volution) was thrown out of function, the power of expression by spoken words was lost. For this reason, the name of "speech-centre" has been applied to this convolution. Since this discovery which links the neurology of the first pari of the cen- tury with that of to-day, and also forms a fundamental observation in the modern doctrine of cerebral physiology, many steps have been taken. It was early observed that although in such cases the capacity for spoken language was lost, nevertheless the muscles which were used in the act of phonation were by no means paralyzed. This relation is due probably to the fact that the muscles are innervated from both hemispheres and possibly also 1 Flechsig : /.'»•. at. Vol. II.— 17 L'.-.S AN AMERICAN TEXT-ROOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. from localities outside the third frontal gyrus. Experiments show that in animals stimulation of the cortex in the region corresponding to the third frontal gyrus causes contractions of many of the muscles employed in speech.1 fhe interesting observation was also made that in the normal right-handed pei-oii the muscles of phonation could not he co-ordinated tor speech from the right hemisphere alone. Thus the symmetrical portion of the right hemisphere has not the same physiological value. Besides this lesion, which involves the cortex in front of the motor region proper, numerous other lesions — namely, those which involve the tracts run- ning between the areas of special sensation (vision and hearing,for example), and the motor or expressive region — produce corresponding disturbances (see Fig. 1T2). An individual in whom the association tracts between the visual and motor areas have been interrupted can, for instance, see an object presented to him in the sense that he gets a visual impression ; hut because of the interruption of the association fibres the object is not recognized, and the impulses reach- ing this sensory area (licit no re- sponse from those muscles the motor centres for which are located outside of the receiving cortex. Upon attempting to picture the anatomical arrangement in anything like the completeness demanded by the physiological reactions, it is necessary to postulate the existence of asso- ciation pathways between each area, whether sensory or motor, Pig. 112. Lateral view of a human hemisphere; cor- ■ . tical ana V, damage to which produces "mind-blind- '-^^i all the others. J Ills arrange- ness"; cortical area //. damage to whirl, produces ment is to be regarded as modi- " mind-deafness " ; cortical area S, damage to which the loss of audible speech; cortical area If, dam- fied in several Ways, age to which abolishes the power of writing. Jn t]|(. firgt plaC6j t])(, (.onnec_ tion between a given sensory and a given motor area differs widely accord- ing to the area.- concerned. The connection, for example, between the visual area and the motor area for the arm is probably represented by more nerve- element-, and these better organized, than the connection between the gusta- tory area and that for the movements of the leg. When, therefore, it i- -aid that such connections exist, it must he added always that the nexus i> different for the several regions concerned, and what i- more, that in man, at least, it i- different for the two hemispheres. Relative Importance of the Two Hemispheres. — The cerebral cortex Semon ami Horsley : Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, 1890, vol. 181. Also -. medicinische \Vo<-!i<-n-. .".]. lx'.Hi. CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. 259 is always active daring our periods of consciousness, and it is to be thought of as a region over which the local point of intensest activity is continually shifting — this focal point, wherever it may be, having about it a halo of less active cells as extensive as the cortex itself. When the subject is right-handed, it appears that injury to the left cere- bral hemisphere is productive of more disturbance than injury to the right hemisphere. At the same time, lesion of the left hemisphere is far more fre- quent than that of the right. So far as can be judged from experiments on man, the higher sense-organs, the eye and the ear, are more perfect, physio- logically, on the right side. Since the connection of the sense-organs is largely with the cortex of the contra-lateral hemisphere, this means that the impulses going mainly to the left hemisphere are better differentiated than those going to the right, and it would appear to be easier for these impulses to reach a motor area in the same hemisphere than to reach the corresponding area on the opposite side. It is further true that in right-handed persons the cortical activities of the left hemisphere in the region of the body sense-area, must always be greater than those of the opposite hemisphere, and these two circumstances cannot fail to have a profound influence. The observations of Flechsig1 on the pyramidal tracts also show that these tracts, before medulla- tion at least, may be unevenly developed on the two sides of the cord, and the ease of control may thus be rendered unequal — a condition which must be dominant in the. determination of the side of the body which shall be most exercised. Be this as it may, the lesions which cause aphasia or apraxia (inability to determine the meaning and use of objects), are predominantly in the left hemisphere in persons who are right-handed, while there is some evidence that the right hemisphere is more important in left-handed persons. In the adult, damage to one hemisphere is usually followed by a perma- nent loss of function, to a greater or less degree, but this loss maybe more transient and less serious when the lesion occurs in the very young subject. so that during the growing period the sound hemisphere can in a measure replace the one that has been injured. Assuming this general plan for the arrangement of the cortex to be cor- rect, it is evident that a, given cell, the axone of which forms part of the pyramidal tract, must in the human cortex be subject to a large series of impulses coming to it over as many paths. Schematically, it would be as represented in Fig. 1 1 .->>. The discharging cell may be destroyed ; then, of course, the muscles con- trolled by it become paralyzed for voluntary movements. The discharging cell may, however, remain intact, but the pathways by which impulses arrive at it be damaged. This is the type of lesion which produces symptoms oi aphasia. When an interruption of associative pathways occurs some one or more of these tracts is broken, and hence the discharging cell doe- not receive a stimulus adequate to cause a response. 1 Leitung8bahnen im Oehri/n und Riickenmark, 1876. 260 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. The physiological complexity of the elements in any part of the central system, either when different portions of the system from the same animal or when the corresponding portions of different animals are compared, depends on the number of paths by which the impulses are brought to the discharging cells. Composite Character of Incoming- Impulses. — To these conclusions based on the anatomy are to be added others suggested by clinical observa- tions. In order that a patient suffering from a lesion between the visual and motor ureas may be able to recognize an object and to indicate its use, it is sometimes necessary that the object shall appeal to several senses. For example, the name and use of a knife, when seen alone, may not be recalled, Fig. 118. — Schema showing in a purely formal manner the different sort of afferent impulses which may influence the discharge of a cortical cell. but when it is taken into the hand — that is, when the dermal and muscular sensations are added to the visual on< — the response is made, though, acting alone, any one -et of sensations is inadequate to produce this result. Just where the block occurs in such a case it is not possible to say with exactness, hut the lesion lies, as a rule, between the sensory and motor areas concerned, and by the damage to the pathway, it is assumed that one or more groups of impulses are so reduced in intensity that they are alone insufficient to produce a reaction ; and therefore it is only when the impulses from several sources are combined that a response can he obtained. Variations in Association. — It is a familiar fact that individuals differ CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. 261 in no small degree in the acuteness of their senses — i. e., in the power to dis- criminate small differences, and this, too, when the sense-organs are normal. Further, the powers of those best endowed arc by no means to be attained br- others, however conscientious their training. Moreover, the sensory path- ways differ widely. The inference is fair, therefore, that those who think in terms of visual images, as compared to those who think in auditory images, do so by virtue of the fact that in the former case the central cells concerned in vision are distinctly the better organized, while in the latter case it is those concerned in hearing. In the same way, the power of expression varies in an equally marked degree, and the capacity for the expression of ideas by means of the hand, in writing, is by no means necessarily equal to the power of expression by means of spoken words. In the former case we have the results of the play of impulses from the several sensory centres on the motor area for the hand, and this is reinforced by the sight of that which has been written, whereas in the latter case impulses from these same sensory centres play upon the area which controls the muscles of phonation, and this reaction is reinforced by the sound of the words uttered. Of course, in the case of a defective, like a blind-deaf- mute, the expression of thought is by movements of the fingers, and this is reinforced by the tactile and muscular sensations which follow these move- ments. It is not by any means to be expected that the anatomical connections which render such reactions possible will be equally perfect for the different sensori-motor combinations, or the same combinations in different persons, and hence the powers of the individual will be modified by the varying per- fection of these paths. From this it also follows that the same lesion as grossly determined, will not produce identical results in the two persons, for it will not effect the damage of structural elements which are strictly com- parable. Latent Areas. — It has been plain from an examination of the foregoing figures, as well as from the descriptions, that there must be a large portion of the cortex which, so far as has been observed, may be called latent. These areas, which include nearly the entire ventral surface of the hemispheres, a large part of the mesial surface, and on the dorsal and lateral aspects a large portion of the frontal, parietal, and temporal lobes together with the island, certainly require a word. These last correspond with the "association centres" as described by Flechsig. To direct stimulation they give no response. From any one portion of the latent area the connections are not massive enough to permit of impulses which will cause a muscular contraction, and hence these impulses coming from one locality to a discharging cell form only a fraction of the impulses which control it. For this reason the significance of these parts fails to be clearly evident upon direct experiment. The cortex of the frontal lobes has some connections with the nuclei of the pons, and so with the cerebellum. The more recent experiments on th« 262 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. functions of this region arc by Bianchi ' and Grosglik,2 the former on monkeys and dogs and the latter on dogs alone. These experimenters found that the removal of one frontal lobe is com- paratively insignificant in its effects, while when both arc removed the change is profound. < )n removing the frontal lobe on one side only there is no dis- turbance of vision, hearing, intelligence, or character. There do occur both sensory and motor disturbances, but these are for the most part transient. On the side opposite to the lesion there is in the limbs a blunting of all sen- sations and some paresis. .Moreover, there is a hyperesthesia combined with a paresis of the muscles of the neck and trunk which move these parts away from the side of the lesion. These several effects of the operation tend to pass off, and if then the remaining frontal lobe be removed from a dog or monkey, not only do the symptoms ju>t described appear on the other side of the body, but still more fundamental changes occur. A ceaseless wandering to and fro, such as Goltz3 observed, in those dogs in which the anterior half of the brain had lien) removed, characterizes the animals; curiosity, affection, sexual feeling, pleasure, memory, and the capacity to learn are at the same time abolished, and the expressions of the animal are those of fear and excessive irritability. That, therefore, the frontal lobe- play an important role in the total reactions of the central system is amply evident, but this by no means justifies the conclusion that they are the seat of the intelligence. F. Comparative Physiology of the Divisions of the Encephalon. For the better comprehension of the conditions found in man and the monkeys, it will be of importance to briefly review the comparative physi- ology of the parts of the encephalon in vertebrates below the monkeys. The encephalon in the lower vertebrates is usually composed of a very much smaller number of cells than is found in that of man, and also the massing of the elements toward the cerebral cortex and in connection with the princi- pal sense-organs has gone on to a tin- less extent. For the determination of the functions of the several parts of the enceph- alon it i- possible to employ in animals the method of removal as well as the method of stimulation. The doctrine of cerebral localization was at one time crudely expressed by the statement that a cortical centre was one the stimu- lation of which produced a given reaction, and the removal of which abolished this same reaction. Goltz soon showed that in the dog the removal of even an entire hemisphere did not cause a paralysis of the muscles on the opposite side of the body, although others had shown that a stimulation of certain portion- of the cortex of the hemisphere would cause the muscles to contract. It was argued, therefore — and quite rightly — that the cortical centres of the dog did not completely answer to the definition. 1 Archives italiennea de Biohgie, 1895, t. xii. 2 Archiv fur Anatomic mid Physiologic, 1895. 3 Ueber dieVerichtungen des Grosshirns, Is- 1. CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. 263 From the experimental work of the strict localizationists like Hitzig,1 Munk,2 and Ferrier,3 and from the work of those who, like Goltz4 and Loci)/' denied a strict localization in the cerebral cortex, several important points of view have been developed. In the first instance, anatomy indicates that in the central system there- are but few localities which consist only of one set of cell-bodies, together with the fibres coming to these bodies and going from them. Almost every part has both more than one set of connections with other parts and also fibres passing through it, or by way of it, to other localities. Hence in re- moving any part of the hemispheres, for instance, not only are groups of cell- bodies taken away, but a number of other pathways are interrupted at the same time, and thus the damage extends beyond the limits of the part re- moved. Moreover, when any portion of the central system has been removed there is a greater or less amount of disturbance of function following imme- diately after the operation ; but this disturbance partially passes away. There are thus "temporary" as contrasted with "permanent" effects of the lesion, and these require to be sharply distinguished, because it is a per- manent loss which is alone significant in these experiments. Finally, it has been made clear that neither the relative nor the absolute value of any division of the central system is fixed, but depends on the degree to which centralization has progressed, or, to use the more common measure, the grade of the animal in the zoological series, both expressions implying an increase in the connections between the cerebrum and the lower centres. The age of the animal on which the operation has been made is also of no small impor- tance in this respect. These relations can be illustrated by reference to several experiments. Removal of Cerebral Hemispheres. — If from a bony fish the cerebral hemispheres (including the corpora striata as well as the mantle) be removed, the animal apparently suffers little inconvenience. The movements are un- disturbed ; such fish play together in the usual manner, discriminate between a worm and a bit of string, and among a series of colored wafers to which they rise always select the red ones first.6 In these fish the eye is the con- trolling sense-organ, ami, as will be recognized (see Fig. 114), the operation has by no means damaged the primary centres of vision. Quite different is the result when the cerebrum is removed from :i shark.' In this case, although the eyes are intact, the animal is reduced to complete quiescence; yet, on the whole, the nervous system of the shark is rather less well organized and more simple than that of the bony fish. The astonishing effect produced is explained by a second experiment (see Fig. 115). 1 Untersuehungen ueber daa Oehirn, Berlin, 1874. 2 Ueber die Funettonen der Orosshimrinde, Berlin, 1881. 3 The Functions i if tin- llrdhi. London, 1S7(>. * Ueber die Veriehtungen dea Gfrosshirns, Bonn, 1881. 8 Archiv fur die gesnmmte Physiologic, 1884, Bde. 33 a. 34. 6 Steiner : Die Functional der Centralnervensystems, 1888. 'Steiner: Loe. cit. 264 AN AMERICAN TEXT- BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. If the olfactory trad be severed on one side, no marked disturbance in the reactions of the shark is to be noticed; when, however, both tracts are severed, the shark acts as though deprived of its cerebrum. From this it appears that the removal of the principal sense-organ, that of smell, is the Pig. 1U.— Schema of the encephalon of a bony fish— embryonic i Edinger). The vertical black Hue marks off the structures in front of the thalamus. real key to the reactions, and that the responsiveness of the fish is reduced in the first instance, because in this case it has been deprived of the impulses coming through the principal organs of sense, and in the second, the removal of the cerebrum contains the pathway for the impulses from the olfactory bulbs to the cell-groups which control the cord. Fig. 115.— Schema of the encephalon of a cartilaginous fish ( Edinger). The vertical black line marks off the striatum and pars olfactoria which lie in front of the thalamus. Passing next to the amphibia as represented by the (Vol;, there are several series of observations on the physiological value <>f the divisions of the cen- tral system. Schrader ' finds the following: Removal of the cerebral hemi- spheres only, the optic thalami being uninjured, does not abolish the sponta- neous activity of the frog. It jumps on the land or swims in the water, and 1 Archir fin- die (jemmmte Physiologie, 1S87, Bd. xli. CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. 265 changes from one to the other without special stimulation. It hibernates like a normal frog, retains its sexual instincts, and can feed by catching passing insects, such as flies (see Fig. 116). A frog without its hemispheres is there- fore capable of doing several things apparently in a spontaneous way. Such frogs balance themselves when the support on which they rest is slowly turned, moving forward or backward as the case demands, in order to maintain their equilibrium. In doing this the frog tends first to move the head inthedirec- /V\ i \ UK Fig. 116. — Frog's brain; the parts in dotted outline have been removed : J, brain intact ; /•'. cerebral hemispheres removed ; C, cerebral hemispheres and thalami removed; IK cerebellum removed'; /-.'. two sections through the optic lobes ; /•', two sections through the right half of the bulb (Stein tion opposite to the motion of the support, and then to follow with move- ments of the body. If the optic thalami arc removed (Fig. IK!, C), the power of balancing is lost, because, although movements of the head still occur, those of the body are abolished. A frog thus operated on and depriv sd of the h im- ispheres and thalami exhibits the lack of spontaneity which is usually d iscrib sd as following the loss of the hemispheres alone, but which is not a necessary consequence of this operation, as the preceding experiments show. A frog possessed of the mid-brain and the parts behind it (Fig. 11<>, C) 266 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. will croak when stroked on the back. When the optic lobes have been removed this reaction becomes more difficult to obtain, but it is not necessa- rily abolished, neither is the characteristic fling of the legs in swimming. At the same time, a frog with its optic lobes can direct both its jumping and swimming movements according to light stimuli acting through theeye,jump- ing around and over obstacles which form a shadow in its path, and climbing out of the swimming tank on the lighter side. This power is lost when the optic lobes have been removed. When the anterior end of the bulb (pars commissuralis — Stieda) has been also removed then the frog becomes incessantly active, creeping about and not coming to rest until he has run himself into some corner. Schrader found such frogs capable of clambering over the edge of a box eighteen centimeters high. They are at a loss when the edge of the box lias been finally attained, and vainly reach into space from this position. In the water they swim "dog- fashion,1' but only upon special stimulation do they make a spring. If more of the bulb is removed, the bearing of the frog departs more and more from the normal, and is only temporarily regained in response to strong stimulation : nevertheless, co-ordinated movements can be obtained when the bulb down to the calamus scriptorius has been removed, and only when the movements of the arms are directly affected by the damage of the upper end of the cord does the inco-ordination become constant. A section through the optic lobes at a t Fig. 116, E) puts the frog in a con- dition similar to that following the isolated removal of the lobes, while a sec- tion at I, has the curious effect of causing the animal to move backward upon stimulation of the toes. When the small ridge which forms the cerebellum in the frog has been removed a slight tremor of the leg-muscles and a loss of precision in jumping arc the only defects noted (Fig. 116, />). These results hold for symmetrical removal of the divisions of the encephalon. When the removal is unsym- metrical in the inter-brain, mid-brain, or bulb (Fig. 116 F, a and //), there is more or less tendency to forced positions or forced movements. As a rule, action is most vigorous on the side of the body associated with the greater quantity of nerve-tissue. This relation appears as a natural result of the greater effectiveness of the incoming impulses when entering a larger group of central cells. Indeed, the removal of the different portions of the central system in the frog is accompanied by a progressive loss in responsive- ness, stronger and stronger stimuli being required to induce a reaction. This hold- true down to the anterior end of the bulb, the removal of which, on the contrary, sets free the lower centres, so that the frog becomes incessantly active, dust how this release is effected is not easy to explain, but further removal is again followed by the loss of responsiveness. Passing next to the bird, as represented by the pigeon, the observations of Schrader arc the most instructive.1 The removal of the hemispheres from the bird involves taking away the mantle and the basal ganglia, the chiasm a 1 Archwfur die gesammte Physiologie, 1888, Bd. xliv. CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. 267 and the optic nerves being left intact. For the Hist few days after the operation the bird is in a sleep-like condition. Next the sleep becomes broken into shorter and shorter periods, and then the bird begins walking about the room. From the beginning- its movements are directed by vision ; slight obstacles it surmounts by flying up to them, larger ones it goes around. In climbing, its movements are co-ordinated by the sense of touch, and the normal position of the body is maintained with vigor. The birds which walk about by day remain quiet and asleep during the night. In flying from a high place the operated pigeon selects the point where it will alight, and pre- fers a perch or similar object to the floor. A reaction to sound is expressed by a start at a sudden noise, like the explosion of a percussion-cap. Pigeons without the cerebrum do not eat voluntarily, though the presence of the frontal portions of the hemispheres is sufficient to preserve the reac- tion. In a young hawk slight damage to the frontal lobes abolished for the time the use of the feet in the handling of food, and thus abolished in this way the power of feeding as well as that of standing. With the loss of the cerebrum the pigeon does not lose responsiveness to the objects of the outer world, but they all have an equal value. The bird is neither attracted nor repelled, save in so far as the selection of the points toward which it will fly is an example of attraction. Sexual and maternal reactions both disappear, and neither fear nor desire is evident. In ascending the mammalian series, the removal of the cerebrum becomes a matter of increasing difficulty. The reasons for this are several, and reside in the increasing size of the blood-vessels and the nutritive complications dependent on the increase in the mass of the cerebrum, as well as in the greater physiological importance of this division. Goltz1 has been able by repeated operations to remove the entire cerebrum of a dog, and still to keep the animal alive and under observation for eighteen months, at the end of which time the animal, though in good health, was killed for further exami- nation. This dog was blind, though he blinked when a very bright light was suddenly flashed in his face. He could be awakened by a loud sound, and when awake responded to such sounds, when intense, by shaking the head or ears. This would not, however, be complete proof that lie could hear. The sense of taste was so far present that meat soaked in quinine was rejected after tasting. Tactile stimuli and those involving the muscle sense. as in the case where the animal was lifted, caused him to struggle and to bid' in the direction of the irritation. These reactions were modified according to the locality of the stimulus. The power to make movements expressive of pain was still present. On the motor side the dog was capable of such highly complicated acts as walking, standing, and eating, and in these operations was guided by the muscle-sense and that of contact. The sexual instincts were lost, but the 1 Archiv fur die gesammte Physiologie, Bd. xli. 268 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. animal was excessively active, and became more and more excited when ready to defecate or when hungry. The examination of the brain showed that in front of the mid-brain the important structures had been removed or were degenerated, only small por- tions of the corpora striata remained, mainly parts of the caudal portions of the nucleus caudatus. The frontal portion of the thalamus had been re- moved and the nuclei in the remainder were highly atrophic, so that the defects were due to a removal of rather more than the cerebrum proper. Emotions, feelings, conscious sensations, or the capacity to learn were entirely wanting in this dog, and its reactions were those of a very elaborate machine. If we compare, now, the effects of the removal of the cerebral hemispheres in the bony fish, the pigeon, and the dog, we see that the results of the operation are progressively more disturbing as we pass up the series. In the higher animals the effects are more often fatal, the disturbance imme- diately following is much more severe, the return of function slower, and the permanent loss greater. As a partial exception to the above statements is the observation that after operation the general health of pigeons always declines, and it is not possible to keep them alive more than about six weeks. On the contrary, a dog could be kept in good health for some eighteen months; but there is this difference between the experiments, that the removal in the case of the dog was made by several successive operations. By removal of the cerebrum the higher animal tends to lose just those capacities which best serve to distinguish it from the lower forms. When, therefore, the inquiry is made why the results obtained in the dog are not obtainable in the monkey or in man, there are several replies. In the first place, no such extensive experiments have been made on monkeys of the right age and under equally favorable conditions. If the mature animal is taken, the secondary degenerations are so massive that they certainly cause great disturbance in the remaining part of the system. This is not equivalent to an assertion that the same results could be obtained in the monkey by more extensive experiments, but :i suggestion of one difference behind the results thus far reported. There is no reason for assuming any deep-seated differ- ence in the arrangement of the central system of the highest mammals as compared with that in the lower. Indeed, in some human microcephalic idiots the proportion of sound and functional tissue in the eneephalon is less than one-lburth that found in a normal person ; yet, on the other hand, no normal adult could lose anything like the amount of tissue which is out of function in these microcephalic brains and at the same time live. The central system, therefore, even in man, is to be looked upon as pos- sessed of some power to adapt itself when portions have been lost, but this is most evident when the defect begins early and develops slowly. Keeping the cerebrum -till in view, it is possible to go into further details. In tonus below the monkey the loss of portions of the cerebral cortex from tin; motor area is accompanied by a greater or less paralysis of CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. 269 the muscles represented. This, however, is an initial symptom only, and gradually disappears, though not always with the same completeness. In man, of course, the tendency to recover is least. The anatomical relations behind this difference are the following: The efferent cells in the ventral horns are dominated principally by two set- of impulses, those arriving directly over the dorsal roots of that segment in which they are located, and those coining over the long paths by way of the cerebral cortex and pyramidal tracts. In the lower mammals this second pathway is insignificant, and when interrupted, therefore, the disturbance in the control of the ventral-horn cells is but slight. Passing up the series, however, this pathway tends to become more and more massive and important, as the figures previously given show (see p. 252), until in man and the monkey a damage of it such as is effected by injury to the cortex causes a high degree of paresis if not permanent paralysis, because by this injury a large proportion of the impulses is thus cut off from the efferent cells. It has been previously shown that the cortical areas do not vary according to the mass of the muscles which they control. Experiments also show that it is the fore-limbs which are most disturbed in their reactions when the lesion involves the cortical centres for both fore- and hind-limbs, and this falls under the law that the more highly adaptable movements (/. c, those of the fore-limb as contrasted with those of the hind-limbs) are most under the con- trol of the cortex. If the examination be restricted to the fore-limb alone, it is found that the finger and hand movements or those of the more distal seg- ments are in turn the ones most disturbed. Thus, in the limbs the more distal groups of muscles are those best controlled from the cortex. It fol- lows, then, that for the arm, paralysis of shoulder movements as the result of cortical lesion is least complete, while as we travel toward the extremity of the arm the liability to disturbance of its function as the result of cortical injury increases steadily. Turning now to the "sensory" areas of the cortex, the principles under- lying their physiological significance and connections appear to be similar. The lower the animal in the vertebrate series the more probable that its reac- tions can be controlled by the afferent impulses which have not passed through the cerebral cortex. None of the senses except vision can be analyzed sufficiently to bring out the significance of the subdivisions of the cortical area; hence the illustra- tions are taken from that sense alone. It lias already been shown that without cerebral hemispheres a bony fish can distinguish the colors of wafers thrown on water and discriminate between a bit of string and a worm. In the same case, a frog is able to direct its movements and to catch flies — i, e.} to detect objects in motion and read to them normally. A pigeon can direct its movements in some measure, and even select a special objeel as a perch ; but it is not able to respond to the sight of food Or its fellows, or those objects which might be Supposed to excite the bird to flight. In the dog the vision which remains permits only the response 270 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. of blinking when the eye is stimulated by the flash of a strong light. The progressive diminution in the response which follows visual stimuli in these animals is open to the interpretation that the path by which the impulses may pass over to the cells forming the primary centres intermediate between the sense-organ and the cortex is progressively diminished. Thus, as the pathway to the cortex becomes more permeable, the impulses arriving at the primary optic centres arc in a less and less degree reflected toward the cord. \\ hen, therefore, the cortex has been removed, the reactions taking place by way of it are disturbed in proportion to their normal importance. In the first instance, when the reflection occurs in the primary centres the incoming impulses are distributed toward the cord by paths not known, while in the second they pass from the cortex along the pyramidal tracts. In the cortex of the dog subdivisions of the visual area have been made by Munk.1 He found that the more anterior portions of the visual area were associated with the superior parts of the retina, and the more posterior por- tions with the inferior, while the area in one hemisphere corresponded with the nasal portion of the retina of the opposite eye, and to a less degree with the temporal portion of the retina of the same side. The determination of these relations was made by the removal of parts of the visual area (dogs) and the subsequent examination of the field of vision. It appears, therefore, that the incoming impulses from certain portions of the retina are delivered at definite parts of the cortex, and that when the parts are injured in the dog or higher mammals these impulses are blocked. By stimulation, it will lie remem- bered, Schafer determined similar relations in the monkey. Before leaving the cerebral hemispheres, mention of the fact should be made that still other functions, control of the sphincter ani (Fig. 103), secre- tion of saliva, and micturition, can be roused by the stimulation of the cortex in the appropriate region — namely, in the region where the muscles and glands concerned might be expected to have representation if they followed the gen- eral law of arrangement. Changes in the production a .d elimination of heat from the body follow interference with the motor region of the cerebrum, and the removal of portions of the cortex in this region is followed by a rise in the temperature of the muscles affected and an increased blood-supply to them. In the encephalon, the cerebrum, and especially its outer surface, is the portion the functions of which have been studied. The significance of the other portions of the encephalon can lie far less well determined. The dis- turbances caused by the section and stimulation of the callosum have been studied by Koi'anyi • and by Schafer.3 It was found that complete section of the corpus callosum was not followed by any perceptible loss of function. On the other hand, stimulation of the uninjured callosum from above gave symmetrica] bilateral movements, while if the cortex on one side was removed stimulation of the callosum gave unilateral movements on the side controlled 1 Ueber die Functionen der Orosshirnrinde, Berlin. 1881. 2 Archiv fur Anatomic und Physiologic, lid. zlvii. Brain, 1890. CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. 271 by the uninjured hemisphere. These results seem to corroborate the conclu- sion derived from histological work to the effecl that the Bystem of the eallo- sum is composed only of commissural fibres, and that it sends no fibres directly into the internal capsule of cither side. Concerning the corpora striata and the optic thalami, very little is known. In the case of the corpora striata injury causes in man no permanent defect of sensation or motion, although both forms of disturbance may at the outset be present in the ease of acute lesions. Lesions of the corpora striata cause a rise in body-temperature.1 Following a puncture of one corpus striatum there occurs in rabbits a rise amounting to some 3° C; it begins a few minutes after the operation mid may last a week, but the temperature tends to return to the normal. The most striking feature in these experiments is the very wide effects produced by an extremely small wound, like the puncture of a probe. In the cases where lesion of the striatum on one side causes in man a rise of temperature, it appears mainly on the side of the body opposite the lesion.2 A vaso-motor dilatation occurs over the parts of the body where the temper- ature is high. In less degree a rise of temperature follows injury of the optic thalamus — at least such is the result of experiments on rabbits; but the effect of the lesion is never so marked as in the case of the striatum. Owing to the dis- proportion between the area of the lesion and the extent of the effects, it is difficult to conceive of the anatomical relations which permit the reaction. It is of interest to note, however, that similar relations hold for the vaso-motor centre in the bulb, in which case the vessels supplying a great area are con- trolled by a small group of cells. The difficulty of an anatomical explanation is increased by the fact that Ott 3 enumerates in animals six heat-centres: 1. The cruciate, about the Rolandic fissure ; 2. The Sylvian, at the junction of the supra- and post-Sylvian fissures ; 3. The caudate nucleus ; 4. The tissues about the striatum ; 5. A point be- tween the striatum and the thalamus, near the median line; (!. The anterior mesial end of the thalamus. Thalamus. — In considering the thalamus, we find that the various cell- groups forming it are connected with distinct portions of the cerebral cortex by double pathways — one set of axones having their origin in cell-bodies located in the cortex, and the other in cell-bodies in the subdivisions of the thalamus. The relations between these two divisions have been specially Studied by v. Monakow,4 who finds by experiment that lesion of one part, cither cortex or the thalamic nuclei, is followed by degeneration in the other part, and that the location of the defeneration depends on that of the lesion. Further, it has been observed by Melius'' that the axones passing from the 1 Aronsolm mid S:iolis: Archiv fur die gesammte Physiologie, 1885, Bd, xxxvii.; Richel : Coviptrx nii/lux d,' VAcad. des Sciences, 1884; Ott: /.' ain, lSS'.t, vol. xi. 2 Kaiser: Neurobgisches Cenlralblalt, 1895, No. 10. 'Ott: Loc at. 4 Archiv fur Psychiatric mul Nervenkrankheiten, 1893, Bd. xxvii. 5 Proceedings of the Royal Society, London, 18U4 and 1895. 272 AN AMERICAN TEXT- BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. motor cortex of the monkey toward the thalamus are fibres of smaller calibre than those destined for the pyramidal tract-. Moreover, the studies of Tschermak ' ou the termination of the tracts which continue the dorsal columns of the spinal cord in the interbrain, show an abundant connection of the fibres, especially with the ventral cell-groups of the thalamus. The connection may be either an actual ending of the fibre or a termination by means of collaterals. When these anatomical observations are considered in connection with the differences in the reactions of the frog with and without, its thalami, it appears that cell-groups which increase the responsiveness of the central system must be located here. On the other hand, in the case of Goltz's dog without its fore-brain, the thalami (interbrain) were so largely damaged that it hardly seems possible that they could have been much utilized in the reactions which were made by that animal. Human pathology throws little light on the functions of the thalami — though lesion of it is often accompanied by loss of power to express the emotions through the muscles of the face — a symptom to which attention has been repeatedly drawn. The Cerebellum. — The only other division of the encephalon, the func- tions of which can properly be described apart, is the cerebellum. This portion is among vertebrates almost a> variable in its development as the mantle of the cerebral hemispheres, and in many lish and mammals is asym- metrical in its gross structure. Observation on this subdivision has been carried out in the first instance by Luciani,2 and later by Russell3 and by Ferrier.4 The cerebellum is not concerned with psychical functions. The removal of it docs not cause permanently either paralysis or anaesthesia, but the im- mediate effects of an extensive injury are (in dogs and monkeys) a paresis and analgesia as well as anaesthesia mainly in the hind-legs, and in conse- quence a high degree of inco-ordination in locomotion. A distinct series of symptoms, however, follows injury to this organ, and these are modified according to the locality and nature of the lesion. Removal of one-half (cerebellar hemisphere plus half the vermis) of the cerebellum in the dog causes a deviation outward and downward of the optic bulb on the opposite side, a proptosis of the bulbs on both sides, nystagmus and contracture of the muscles of the neck on the side of the lesion, and an increase of the tendon-reflexes in the limbs, in walking the dog wheels toward the side Opposite lo the lesion, and tends to fall toward the side of the lesion. The symptoms are chiefly unilateral, and, caudad from the cerebellum, are on the side of the lesion. The symptoms are less severe when only one hemisphere, instead of an entire half of the cerebellum, has been removed. 1 "Xutiz betreffi des Rindenfeldes der Hinteretrangsbahnen," XeurologischesCrninilblatt, 1898,, No. 4. 1 Archives italiennes df Biologic, 1891-92, xvi. 1 Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, 1894. * Brain, 1893, vol. xvi. CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. The existing symptoms arc not intensified by the removal of the remaining half. The permanent condition of the muscles after operation is expressed l>v an atonia, or lack of tonus, in the resting muscles ; an asthenia, or loss of strength, which was measured by Luciani,and was most marked in the hind- leg; an astasia, or a lack of steadiness in the muscles during action; and finally an ataxia, or a want of orderly sequence, in the contraction- of the muscle-groups. The general expression of these symptom is a twisl of the trunk, the concavity being toward the operated side, combined with a dis- orderly gait. At the same time there is no demonstrable permanent dis- turbance of tactile or muscular sensibility. Though the two halves of the cerebellum are united by strong commis- sural fibres, the complete division of the organ in the middle line is followed by a disturbance of the gait which is only transitory. Hence it is inferred that the connections of the cerebellum are mainly with the same side of the bulb and spinal cord. Cephalad of the cerebellum the connection, however, is a crossed one, each cerebellar hemisphere being associated with the contra- lateral cerebral hemisphere. Throughout these connections, both cephalad and caudad to the cerebellum itself, it appears that there is always a double pathway, and the cerebellum not only sends impulses to, but receives them from, the regions with which it is associated. One effect of removal of one-half of the cerebellum is to increase the re- sponsiveness of the cortex of the contra-lateral cerebral hemisphere to electri- cal stimulation, thereby making it possible with a weaker stimulus to obtain a reaction which could be obtained from the other hemisphere only with a stronger one. When an irritative lesion is made, instead of a merely de- structive one, the rotation and falling are away from the side of the lesion, instead of toward it. The experiments altogether -how the cerebellum to be closely associated with the proper contraction of the muscles, and this i> so directly connected with the maintenance of equilibrium ' that it is not surprising to find that stimulation or removal of the cerebellar cortex, besides producing nystagmus, may give rise to deviations of the eyes similar to those found on injury of the semicircular canals or stimulation of their nerves in fishes.2 PART III.— PHYSIOLOGY OF TliE NERVOUS SYSTEM TAKEN AS A WHOLE. A. Weight of the Brain and Spinal Cord. In attributing a value to the mass of the nervous system we assume that the elements which compose it possc-s potential energy. This energy varies for any given element in accordance with :i number of conditions, but for the moment it will be sufficient to point out that if the mass of the entire system is significant the masses of its respective subdivisions are also significant, as 1 A. Thomas: " Le Cervelet," F.uu], anafomique, dinigue et physiologique, Paris, 1897. 2 Lee: Journal of Physiology, 1893, vol. sv. ; 1894, vol. wii. Vol. II.— IS ■2:\ AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. showing in some measure the relative physiological importance of the several parts. Weight of the Encephalon and Spinal Cord. — When the weight of any portion of the aervous system is taken, the final record represents, in addi- tion to the weight of the nerve-tissues proper, that of the supporting and nutritive tissues normally associated with them, together with the enclosed blood and lymph. It is, however, assumed that under normal conditions the relation between the nervous and non-nervous tissues is nearly a constant one, and that the results of different weighings are therefore comparable among themselves. Outside of the nervous tissue proper are the pia and the fluid contained in the vessels and ventricular cavities. Sometimes the encephalon is freed from Til.. 117. — Showing tin- principal divisions of the encephalon made for the study of its weight: 1, hemisphere seen from the side, fissuration according to Eberstaller ; 2, mid-brain, region of the quad- rigemina ; 3, pons; 1, cerebellum, or hind-brain ; 5, bulb, or after-brain. Divisions 2, 3, and •'>. taken r, form what is designated the "stem " in the tables of Boyd (modified from Quain's Anatomy). the pia and fluid, and at others they are weighed all together. According to Broca,1 the weight of the pia covering the encephalon is, in normal males, as follows : 20 to 30 years ' 45 gms. 31 to 40 " 50 " 60 " 60 " The cast of the ventricles as made by Welcker displaces 26 c.c. of water, which gives an idea of the normal capacity of these cavities. In man, the gray matter of the cerebrum has, on the average, Si per cent, of water; while the white matter from various parts of the central system has 70 per cent.2 1 Broca, quoted by Topinard: Elements / th< ll>\jnl S„rirhj, London, 1860; see also Marshall : Journal of Anatomy and I'tnjxioloipj, ls'.t'i. 2 Keith: Journal of Anatomy and Physiology, 1895. :; Imi Bois, in the Arch, fur Anthropol., Bd. xxw. maintains that among forms which may be fairly compared, the formula E = S0'56, will give the weight of the encephalon, — E being the encephalic weight and S the body-weight. CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. -J 7, loss of the brain in weight after maturity, observations on animals are scanty, but point to decrease in weight toward the natural close of life. Interpretations of Weight. — Assuming as the simplest case that the number of the nerve-elements composing a given portion of the central system is constant within the limits of the same species, then differences in the weight of these portions in different individuals imply variations in the size of the component cells. The significance of variations in the size of the nerve-ele- ments must be, primarily, that the larger the cells, and especially the larger the cell-bodies, the greater the 'mass of cell-substance ready at any moment to undergo chemical change leading to the release of energy, and the more nu- merous the probable connections. On the other hand, if the number of ele- ments is variable, an increase in the number must, in view of the law of isolated conduction, also provide a larger number of conducting pathways. Whether this increase in the number of pathways shall further add to the complication of the system depends on the localities at which it occurs. In the absence of fuller data, the explanation of the series of differences shown in Boyd's table is in a very high degree tentative. The loss of weight in advanced years appears to be due to a general atrophy of the nerve-ele- ments. The greater brain-weight associated with greater stature appears to depend on the variations in the size of the elements rather than in their num- ber, and, so far as can be seen, the distinction according to sex is also sus- ceptible of the latter explanation. Weights of Different Portions. — A study of the proportional weights of the several subdivisions of the encephalon according to the sex, stature, and age, shows that there is very little difference caused by variations in these conditions. This, too, so far as it goes, suggests that the absolute weight is dependent rather on variations in the size than in the number of the elements, since an harmonious variation in number would be less probable than an har- monious variation in size. Social Environment. — It is not to be expected that the weight of the brain among the least-favored classes in any community will be the same as that of those who, during the years of growth, are under favorable conditions. All extensive series of observations which we possess relate to the leasts- favored social classes, and hence it is not improbable that the figures id the foregoing tables, which are based on data obtained mainly at the Marylebone Workhouse in London, are decidedly below those which would be obtained from the more fortunate classes in the same community. We have a li>t "t brain-weights which contains the records for a number of men of acknowl- edged eminence, and also for others who attained recognition as able persons without being exceptionally remarkable. This list shows the persons thus selected to have brains on the average heavier than the usual hospital sub- ject.1 Brain-weight of Criminals. — The observations of Manouvrier have shown that among French murderers the brain-weight is similar to that of the indi- 1 Donaldson: The Orowth of the Brain, 1895. 278 AN AMERICAN TEXT- HOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. viduals usually examined in the Parisian hospitals. In the same manner, the observations on the h rain-weight among the insane indicate, according to the records of Boyd and others, that the insane as a class (the microcephalics being excluded) are not characterized by a special brain-weight. When, however, the insane are grouped according to the form of disease from which they have suffered, it is evident that those in which the brain was congested at death exhibit the higher weight, while those in which the pathological processes caused destructive changes, exhibit a low weight. The differences in these case- arc rather the results of disease than the cause of it. Brain-weights of Different Races. — Concerning the weights of the brain in different races there are no extensive observations which have been made directly on the brain itself. Davis,1 however, has determined the cranial capacities of a series of skulls belonging to different races, and the brain- weights have been calculated from these.2 This calculation gives the largest brain-weights to the western Europeans, but for a proper interpretation of the results there are needed at least the data concerning stature and age of the cases studied, both of which are here lacking. Weight of the Spinal Cord. — Comparatively few observations are avail- able for the spinal cord : Mies3 found that in adults it weighed 24 to 33.3 grams, with an average weight of 26.27 grams ; this for the cord deprived of the nerve-roots but covered by the pia. The variations due to sex and stat- ure have not been determined. It seems probable, however, that the cord, like the brain, will be found lighter in females and in short persons: Mies states that its decrease in old age i> proportionately less than that of the brain. B. Growth-changes. The characters of the brain and cord thus far described have been those found in the adult. Between birth and the natural end of life, however, great changes take place, and, as it is necessary to consider the functions of the central system at all times in its history, the importance of knowing the direction in which the growth-changes are probably occurring is obvious. Growth of the Brain. — The weight of the brain from birth to the twenty-fifth year is shown in Fig. 118. The curve is based on the table of Vierordt.4 The curve beyond the twenty-fifth year is continued on the basis of the observations by Bischoff,8 and for comparison the curve representing the en- cephalic weights of a series of eminent men, forty-five in number, is drawn in a dotted line, the averages for decennial periods being alone dotted. These records exhibit the fact that at birth the weight of the brain is about one-third of that which it will attain at maturity. The increase is very rapid 1 Journal of the Academy of Natural Sciences, Philadelphia, 1*69. 2 Donaldson: Growth of the Brain, 1895, p. 115. 3 Neurologischea Centralblatt, L893. * Archivfur Anatomic »>"/ Physiologic, 1890. Hirngewicht da Menschen, Bonn, 1^80. CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. •279 during the first year, and vigorous for the first seven or eight years, after which it becomes comparatively slow. The maximum weight is indicated in the fifth decade (males), fourth (females), although there is a "premaxi- mum" in the middle of the second decade (at thirteen and fifteen years for males and fourteen years for females), in which the too early and too vigorous 280 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. growth of the encephalon appears to be an important factor in the cause of death ; hence the larger brain-weight found at autopsies during these years. While, in general, the individual may be supposed to follow, in the develop- ment of his encephalon, the course here indicated by the curve, this premax- imal increase must be excepted for the reasons given. Relation between Growth of Body and that of Encephalon. — On com- paring the growth of the entire body with that of the encephalon, it is evi- dent that the growth is more rapid in the central nervous system than in the body at large, and that it is almost completed in the former at the end of the eighth year, whereas the body has at that time reached but one-third of the weight which it will attain at maturity. A causal relation between a well-developed central system and the subse- quent growth of the entire body is thus suggested, and also it is evident that conditions which influence growth will at any time find the body on the one hand, and the central system on the other, at quite different phases in their development. The long-continued growth of the body brings it about that the central system, which at birth may form 12 per cent, of the total weight of the indi- vidual, is at maturity about 2 per cent, or less. For this change in propor- tion the increase of the muscular system is mainly responsible. Further, the much smaller mass of the muscular system in the female is the chief cause of the higher percentage value of the central system in the female — a relation which has been much emphasized, but which is really not signifi- cant, since iu both sexes this high percentage value of the central system is mosi developed at birth, and becomes steadily less marked as maturity is approached. Increase in the Number of Functional Nerve-elements. — Having thus briefly indicated the facts of growth so far as they can be detected by the balances, it still remains to mention the series of changes which may be studied bv other means, such as micrometric measurements or enumeration. The results obtained by these methods are somewhat complex, and must be treated with great care. Human embryology indicates that after the third month of fetal life the number of cells in the central system is not increased. With the cessation in the production of new cells the only remaining means of increase in size is by enlargement of those cells already present. How this occurs is well indicated by the accompanying table (p. 281), which -hows the change in the size of cell -bodies in a given locality in man. All vertebrates are not similar in respect to the manner of this change. I>irge ' has shown that in frogs there is a gradual increase in the number of the fibres forming the ventral and dorsal spinal roots, and that this goes on at the rate of about fifty additional fibres in the ventral roots and seventy in the dorsal, for each gram added to the total weight of the frog. The increase was still apparent in a frog weighing 112 grams. In the case of the ventral 1 Birge : Archivfur Anatomie und Phyxiologie, suppl., 1S82. CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. 281 root-fibres it was also determined that the cells of origin in the ventral horns of the spinal cord increased at a corresponding rate Here is exemplified an instance of long-continued enlargement of the nervous system by the regular development of immature cells, a method of growth most marked probably in those animals which increase in size so long as they live. Volumes of the Largest Cell-bodies in the Ventral Horn of the Cervical Cord of Man (based on Kaiser's records of the mean diameters). The volume TOO,"3, in the fetus of four weeks, is taken from His, and the figures represent multiples of that volume. Subject. Fetus Child at birth . . Boy at fifteen years Man, adult . . 4 weeks 20 " 24 " 28 " 36 " Proportional volume of the cell-bodies 1=70(V3. n 17 I 31 \ 67 | 81 j 124} 124 f 160 Time interval. 36 weeks. 15 years. 15 " It is believed that in this case the new cells and new fibres are not, strictly speaking, new morphological elements, but are the result of devel- opmental changes taking place in the cells present in the system from an early period. A distinction is thus to be made between cell-elements which, because they are not developed, are therefore not a part of the system already physio- logically active, and those cells already organized together and which are fully functional. When, therefore, it is said that the cells of origin for the ventral root-fibres increase in number, the increase refers to the latter group, and not to the total number of elements of both kinds present in the cord. In other words, the number of cells appears to increase because the number of developed cells becomes greater. In support of this suggestion the observations on the growth of the fibres in the roots of the frog's spinal nerves maybe cited.1 Hardesty found the greatest number of medullated fibres in the ventral roots, nearest the cord, and in the dorsal roots, nearest the spinal ganglion. inns in each the greatest number was nearest the cells of origin, an arrangement which is most readily explained by assuming that some of the fibres had grown but a short distance from their cells of origin at the time the frog was killed. On the other hand, Schiller-' counted the number of nerve-fibres in the oculo-motor nerves of cats, and found but a very slight difference in this number between birth and maturity. So far, then, as this nerve is concerned, it is found in the cat to be nearly complete at the time of birth. In man there are very lew observations on the increase in the number of 'Hardesty: Journal of Comparative Neurology, 1899, vol. ix. 2 Schiller: Comples rendus de VAcadtm.it des Sciences, I'uris. L889. 282 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. functional nerve-cells with age. Kaiser,1 as is shown in the accompanying table, found in man increasing numbers of large nerve-cells in the ventral horns of the spinal cord at the ages named : Number of Developed Cells in the Cervical Enlargement of Man at Different Ages (Kaiser). Age. Number of Nerve-cells. Fetus, 10 weeks 50,500 " 32 " 118,330 New-born child 104,270 Boy, 15 years 211,800 Male, adult 221,200 Here, as in the frog, the apparent increase must be looked upon as due to the gradual development of elements present from an early date. And it must be further remembered that in this case the cells thus maturing after birth probably belong in a large measure to the group of "central cells," the function of which is to increase the complexity of the pathways within the cord. Fig. IK". Diagram illustrating the extent of the cerebral cortex. The outer square i,* , shows a sur- face approximately one-fiftieth of 2352 sq. cm. in extent ; the inner square (.1) has two-thirds of this area, and is the proportion of the cortex sunken in the fissures. 2352 sq. cm. are approximately the areaof the entire cortex in a male brain weighing 1360 grams. Increase in the Fibres of the Cortex. — The area of the cerebral cortex (see Fig. 11!)) varies according to several conditions, but in general the more voluminous the cerebral hemispheres the greater it- extent. That which covers the walls of the sulci, — the sunken cortex — has in man about twice the extent of that directly exposed on the surface of the hemispheres. In the cortex of the human cerebral hemispheres it has been shown by Vulpius2 that the number of fibres in the different layers is greater in the 1 DU Functional der Oanglienzellen dea Haismarkes, Haag, 1891. -' Vulpius: Archiv fur Psychiatric und Nervenkrankhcilen , 1892. CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. 283 thirty-third year than at earlier periods, but in old age this number is decreased. At exactly what age decrease sets in, is not to be determined from these observations. They show, simply, that in general the number of fibres was less at seventy-nine years than at thirty-three years. In a similar way Kaes has shown 1 that the association fibres of the human cerebral cortex form three parallel systems. In general it is the deepest layer — i.e., that farthest from the surface of each system — which first becomes medullated. The first fibres appear at about the fourth month of life in the deepest portion of the deepest layer. The middle system is the last to be completely medullated, this process continuing in it up to the forty-fifth year of life. Passow 2 has shown that at maturity the cortex of the central gyri exhibits association fibres which increase in abundance as we pass from the great longitudinal fissure (leg area) toward the Sylvian fissure, these fibres being most abundant in the areas for the hand and fingers. On the other hand, in the central gyri of a child fifteen months of age, these fibres are equally abundant in these two localities. From this it appears that the differentia- tion takes place after the first year of life. Significance of Medullation. — Two sorts of nerve-fibres are described — those with and those without a medullary sheath. Both have the power of isolated conduction, but in the peripheral system the non-medullated fibres are found in connection with the sympathetic system, where less specialized functions are carried on, and also in a large but varying degree in the central system. The wider significance of this difference in medullation is at the moment quite obscure. The first suggestion, that absence of the medullary sheath is an immature con- dition which persists in various parts of the nervous system, brings us at once to the question of the physiological difference thus implied, but not explained. It is known that the central system is at birth very imperfectly medul- lated, and the growth of these medullary sheaths must form a large part of the total increase in its bulk. In the mature nerve-fibre the axis-cylinder and the medullary sheath have nearly equal volumes, and therefore approxi- mately equal weights. The medullated fibres form probably not less than 97 per cent.3 of the total weight of the nerve-tissues composing the encepha- lon, and of this nearly one-half would be medullary substance. Increase in the Mass of the Neurones. — The amount of this increase under various conditions has already been discussed, and been found to range between zero and fifty-thousand-fold (p. 17(i>. Number of Cells. — Any attempt to determine the entire number of nerve-cells, the bodies of which lie within the wall.- of the neural tube, must be open to many sources of error. LKaes: Wiener med. Woehenschrifl, 1895, NTos. 41,42; Kaes: Jahrbiichern der Hamburg, Stoats JSsankenanstallen, J ahrgang, 1893-94, Bd. iv. 2 Passow: Archivfiir Psychiatrie, Bd. 31, 8. 859,860. 'Thompson: Journal of Comparative Anatomy, W'.i, vol, i\.: Donaldson: Ibid., No. '2. 284 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. A careful study of the cortex1 based on Hammarherg's records, gives 9200 million cell-bodies in this region alone. Considering the amount of gray matter present in the rest of the central system, an estimate of 13,000 million for the total Dumber in the entire central nervous system is probably a conservative calculation. From the foregoing tacts, together with those bearing on the cell-elements, it is possible to get some conception of the growth-processes in the central system, and to see how they are due to an enlargement of the nerve-elements which have been formed at a very early stage in the life-history of the indi- vidual. In such enlargements the chief increase is due to the formation of the axones, and in them, in turn, about half the substance is represented by the medullary sheaths. In all probability these sheaths are no exception to the rule according to which all parts of the body are variable, not only in their absolute, but also in their relative size, and therefore it is possible that the quantitative variation in this constituent is a very important factor in modifying the weight of the central system, perhaps accounting in some cases for the very heavy brains occasionally reported. Change in Specific Gravity with Age. — During fetal life and at birth the percentage of water in the nerve-tissues is high, but becomes less at maturity. In white rats at birth the percentage of water for the encephalon is 89 per cent, and for the spinal cord 85.3 per cent. At maturity it is about 78 per cent, for the encephalon and 70.1 per cent, for the cord. This change is correlated in some measure with the development of the medullary sub- stance. For the gross physical changes which have thus been indicated as occur- ring during growth, an explanation is to be found in the changes affecting the constituent elements, and these have been set forth when describing the growth of the individual cells. C. Organization and Nutrition of the Central Nervous System. What is here meant by organization maybe easily illustrated. When, for example, by later growth new tissue is added to the liver, or the skin is in- creased in area or a muscle enlarged, there is caused by the addition of new substance a change in the powers of these tissues, which is mainly quantita- tive, flie larger organ exhibits the same capabilities that the smaller organ exhibited, but does so in a greater degree. In the central nervous system, on the other hand, it appears that with growth the system becomes capable of new reactions in the sense that its various responses are controlled and directed bv a larger number of incoming impulses, and thus the number, complexity, and refinement of the reactions are increased, and in this sense it really attains new powers. \\ ith the change in the age of the central system there occurs from birth up to the prime of life, if we may judge from general reactions, an increase 1 Thompson: Journal of Comparative Neurology, 1899, vol. ix. CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. 285 in this organization. This is maintained for a time, and then in old age it breaks clown, at first gradually, and later rapidly. It becomes important, therefore, to examine the manner in which this organization is accomplished. Organization in the Central System. — When first formed the cells com- posing the central system are completely separated from one another. In the mature nervous system the impulses, as has been pointed out, probably travel for the most part from the axones of one unit to the dendrites of another. For organization the most important changes, however, are those affecting the cell outgrowths, both dendrites and axones. During growth both of these increase in the length of their main stems and of their respective branches. In picturing the approach of two elements within the central system the pro- cess is usually described as that of the outgrowth of the axone toward the dendrites or bodies of those cells which are destined to receive the impulse, but it must not be forgotten that the dendrites are also growing, and the question of the approximation of the branches of these latter to those of the axone depends in part on their own activities. The conditions modifying this process are, however, obscure. It is evi- dent that medullation outside of the central system is not necessary to the functional activity of a fibre, and therefore probably in the central system unmedullated fibres are also in many cases functional. Whatever may be the relation of the establishment of new pathways to the acquisition of medul- lary sheaths by the axone and its branches, it is also found that all fibres which when mature are medullated begin as unmedullated fibres, and that the increase in medullation throughout the central system is an index of the increase in organization. A consideration of the facts of growth in the layers of the cortex, for instance, will show them to be open to this interpretation. Applying these ideas concerning organization to the three classes of cells, afferent, central, and efferent, which compose the nervous system, we find the following: In the central system the afferent cells contribute to organization by the multiplication of the collaterals. At the periphery the division of the branches of the axone increases the number of opportunities for excitation which such an element oilers. These cells are, lor the most part, without dendrites. Among the central cells all possible modes of growth are con- tributory ; that is, the branches of both kinds add directly to the complexity of the central pathways. On the other hand, the efferent group contribute- to this complexity almost solely by the lbrniati< f dendrites, the collaterals which come from the axones of these cells forming but an insignificanl con- tribution. Not only, therefore, is organization in large part dependent on changes in the central cells by reason of their numerical preponderance, but also by reason of the fact that to them a multiplication <>t' pathways both by elaboration of the axones and the dendrites is alone possible. Defective Development. — In view of these tacts, defective development in the nervous system may depend on failure in one or more of these several processes by which the system is organized, and it should be possible to corre- late defective development involving mainly one set of elements with a dis- 286 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. tinct clinical picture. The results of defective development are not merely an absence of certain powers, 1 >ut in some measure a diminution in the strength and range of those thai remain (Hammarberg *). Laboratory Animals. — The 1 tearing of these facts on the conception which we form of the nervous systems of those animals commonly employed for laboratory experiment.- may be here mentioned. The frog, pigeon, rabbit, cat, and dog form a series in which the total ma^s of the central system in- creases from the beginning to the end of the series. The number of cells in the largest system, that of the dog, is many times greater than that in the smallest, the frog; and it is probable that the others are in this respect intermediate. Organization is apparently more rapidly completed and more nearly simultaneous throughout the entire system in forms like the frog and pigeon, and also in these latter the organization is least elaborate. While the educability of the nervous system of the dog may depend on several conditions, the comparative slowness of organization is undoubtedly one of them, and a very important one. Where the organiza- tion is early established it is also simple, and thus portions of the system retain through life a greater capacity for acting alone. In selecting an ani- mal, therefore, on which to make a series of experiments, these several facts must be kept in view, for the choice is by no means a matter of indifference. Blood-supply. — For the general distribution of the blood-vessels in rela- tion to the gross subdivision of the brain the student is referred to the works on anatomy. The finest network of vessels is, however, to be found where the cell-bodies are most densely congregated, and indeed the distinction between the masses of gray and white matter in the central system is as clearly marked by the relative closeness of the capillary network as in any other way (see p. 191). One result of this relation between the blood-sup- ply and the cell-bodies which form the gray matter is a general arrangement of the vessels along the radii of the larger subdivisions of the brain, as the cerebral hemispheres and the cerebellum. The conditions which control the circulation within the cranium and spinal canal are not exactly the same at all periods of life, but the variations occur in minor points only. The studies of IIuberJ show that in the cat, dog, and rabbit at least, the vessels in the pia of the cerebral hemispheres are supplied with both medul- lated and unmedullated nerves. The former are probably sensory in func- tion ; the latter, possibly, vaso-motor. These latter nerves have been fol- lowed to arteries so small as to possess but two layers of muscle-cells, but were not traced by Huber to vessels actually penetrating the nervous sub- stance of the hemispheres, von Kolliker, however, claims to have followed them even there. These observations make the existence of a corresponding vaso-motor 1 Bammarberg: Studien ueber Klinik und Pathologie der ff the fluids within, and hence tends to facilitate nutrition during the earlier Btages of growth. In pathological eases where the cranial wall has been destroyed there is a similiar variation in volume to be observed in the adult, and it is possible that the beneficial effects which in so many instances follow trephining of 1 Howell: American Journal of I'livsinh^n/, lS'JS, i. \(). 1. 1 Hill : Journal >>f Physiology, 1895, v<>l. xviii. 3 W. und Ed. Weber: Mechanikder memchlichen Qehwerkzeuge, 183G. 288 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. the >kull may depend upon this mechanical release. Of course, in cases with a defective skull-wall an increase in arterial pressure causes a more decided increase in the volume of blood in the brain ; this, however, is much more marked than it would be under ordinary conditions, and is not to be regarded as the main effect, which is an increase in the quantity of the blood passed through the central system in a unit of time. Mosso ' has found the tempera- ture of the blood coming from the brain (dog's) slightly higher than that of the rectum and of the arterial blood. The differences are very small, but he draws the conclusion that the metabolic processes in the brain are sufficiently intense to raise the temperature of the blood passing through it. A.S against the intensity of the metabolism in the central system, it has been observed that blood taken from the torcular Herophili of the dog was Intermediate in gaseous content between arterial blood and that taken from the femoral vein, thus indicating that the arterial exchange was less intense in the brain than in the muscles of the leg. The following is a condensed statement of the figures : Percentages <>/ Oxygen and Carbonic Acid in virions Samples of Dogs' Blood , c ... , . , . i <<>.,. . 37.64 percent Average oi 52 arterial samples lO 18 25 " a r aoi i i fC02. . 41.65 " Average of 42 torcular samples lO 13 49 " Average of 28 femoral vein samples - -' „'L. The absolute quantity of the blood in the brain and cord is certainly small ; if we may judge from the observations on animals, it is not more than 1 per cent, of the entire blood in the body. It is to remembered, how- ever, that the cell-bodies, which alone are well supplied with blood, probably represent less than 2 per cent, of the entire encephalic mass. With general rise and fall of pressure elsewhere, there is a rise and a fall of pressure within the central system. During the first phases of mental activitv blood is withdrawn from the limbs ; the blood thus withdrawn can be shown to pass toward the trunk' and head, for when a person lying on a horizontal table supported at the centre on a transverse knife-edge is just balanced, then increased activity of the cerebral centres causes the head to dip down (Mosso), and if the skull-wall is defective the brain is seen to swell. In the later stages of fatigue the blood-supply to the nerve-centres dimin- ishes owing to a decrease in force of the heart-beat and the tonicity of the splanchnic vessels, SO that the brain in birds exhausted by a long flight has been found by Mosso to be in a high degree anaemic. There is much reason to think that in man a similar reaction occurs. The study <>f the cerebral circulation in the ease of those in whom the skull-wall is at sonic point deficient shows a bulging of the skin over the 1 I>i> Temperatur des Gehirns, Leipzig, 1894. 2 Journal of Physiology, 1895, vol. xviii. CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. 289 opening into the cranial cavity as a result of mental effort or emotion. In the normal adult this bulging cannot, of course, occurto anything like such an extent, and the space for the arterial blood must he gained both by driving out the blood from the cerebral veins within the cranium and through the expulsion of the subdural fluid. Influence of Glands. — In the growth of the nervous system it is not only the quantity, but the peculiar qualities of the blood that are important, and among the various glands the activity of which is so necessary for the growth of the nervous, as well as the other systems, and is also needed for its full maintenance, the thyroid appears as very important. In sporadic cretinism, associated as it is with atrophy of the thyroid, the feeding of sheep's thyroids has produced remarkable growth-changes in all parts of the body — the nerv- ous system included. At the same time, experimental extirpation of the thyroid is followed by destructive changes in the central system, caused by disturbances in its nutri- tion. The future will doubtless reveal other forms of internal secretion which also have a significance for the activity of the central system. Starvation. — In starving animals the nervous system loses but very little in weight.1 This small loss is most striking in view of the fact that exten- sive histological changes occur in the cell-bodies. However, if we consider the cell-bodies as the part mainly affected during starvation, then the small mass of the cell-bodies would go far toward explaining the result, but it does not explain why the myeline is so resistant. Fatigue. — The histological basis of fatigue as expressed by the changes in the individual cells, has already been discussed. The fatigue of the system as a whole is but the expression of fatigue in large numbers of its elements, but the manner in which the changes show themselves is somewhat complicated. When the attempt is made to raise a weight by the voluntary contractions of the muscles of the index finger at regular intervals, say once a second, it is found that if the weight be heavy the power of the finger decreases, and the weight soon ceases to be lifted as high as at first. Finally a point is reached when the voluntary effort produces little or no elevation of the weight. if, however, despite this failure, the effort is still made at regular intervals, it happens, in some persons, that this power returns gradually, and a few sec- onds later the contractions are very nearly as high as at the beginning of the experiment (Mosso). This phenomenon may repeat itself many times, giving a record formed by groups of contractions most extensive near the centre of each group, these latter being separated by portions of the curve in which the contractions are very small or wanting (see Kig. l'JO). (Sec ( Jeneral Physiology of Nerve and Muscle, p. 135.) Daily Rhythms. — Within the cycle of the astronomical day the progress of events leading to fatigue is not a steady one. Lombard'-' found that if the capacity for voluntary effort was measured by the amount of work which 1 Voit: Zeitechrift fiir Biologie, 1894, lid. xx.\. 2 Journal of Physiology, 1892, vol. xiii. Vol.. tr. 19 290 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. could be done by voluntarily contracting the flexor muscles of the index finger before the first failure to respond to a voluntary stimulus appeared, then - — ~\ lil. hill I I I I llllllll I I I III I Fig. 120.— A record of the extent of the flexions of the forefinger lifting a weight at regular intervals. The light lines are those for the voluntary contraction ; the heavy lines, those for contractions following the direct stimulation ofthe flexor muscles by electricity. In the former then- are periods, in the latter none. The arrow shows the direction in which the record is to be read (Lombard i. the curve expressing this capacity for voluntary work throughout the day was represented as in Fig. 121. Briefly, the curve shows two maxima, at 10 P. M., and 10 a. M., with two minima midway between them. In general Fig. 121.— Showing at each hour of the day and night how many centimeters a weight of 3000 grams could be raised by repeated voluntary contractions ofthe forefinger before fatigue sets in. The curve is highest at 10 to 11 a.m. and 10 to 11 p. m.; lowest, 8 to 4 p. m., and 3 to 4 a.m. circle with dots, observa- tion made just after taking food; square with dot, smoking ; *, work done 8 minutes after drinking 15 c.c. of whisky ( Lombard i. the immediate effect of taking food is to increase the work done by the sub- ject. Alcohol lias the same effect, while smoking produces a decrease. Further, from day to day this capacity for work is influenced by a num- ber of external conditions — temperature, barometric pressure, etc. CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. 291 Time Taken in Central Processes. — All processes in the nervous system take time, and are for the most part easy to measure. The rate of the nerve- impulse has already been given. When, however, it passes from one element to another, the delay is even more marked, and it is plausible to assume that this detention occurs at the juncture of the elements. Thus in those parts of the central system where the cell-elements and also the cell-junctions are most numerous, the time taken is longest. Fig. 122 shows this very well. Between the middle of the cerebral hem- isphere and the optic lobes, although the distance is short, the impulse takes twice as long to travel as between the bulb and the lumbar enlargement. When this time is measured in the conscious individual it is, of course, open to a long series of modifying conditions, and these appear to be in part the same conditions which modify the muscular endurance of the individual at different portions of the day. Thus it has been determined that the speed with which reactions can be made as indicated by the reaction time, is subject to variations, and does not steadily decrease from the morning to the evening. Fig. 122.— To show the rate at which impulses pass through the nervous system of a frog. At the extreme left the vertical has the value of 0.5 second and the other verticals are compared with it; thus between the cerebrum and the optic lobe requires about 0.25 second ; between the bulb and the lumbar enlargement a greater distance— only about half the time ; and for the still greater distance represented by the length of the sciatic nerve even less time is needed (Exner). It has been the purpose of the paragraphs just preceding to indicate that through the day it is not possible to demonstrate a steady decline of power in the nervous system. We begin the morning, to be sure, feeling fresh, and are fagged in the evening, but the course by which this condition has been attained is not a simple or direct one. D. Sleep. Conditions Favoring Sleep. — To recover from fatigue sleep is required. The prime condition favoring sleep is the diminution of nerve-impulses pass- ing through the central system. This is accomplished in two ways. In the first instance it is usual to reduce all incoming stimuli to a minimum. This is most directly under our own control. On the other hand, the permeability of the nervous system and the intensity with which it responds arc decreased as the result of the beginning fatigue. I low these conditions arc brought aboul has been a matter of much speculation and some experiment. The parts played by the sensory and that by the central cells vary some- what at different times of life, for impulses arc much less widely diffused in early years than at maturity. Moreover, in childhood the amount of stored material is small, large at maturity, ami small again in old age, and this holde 292 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. true for all the groups of cells. Hence the cells would, by reason of this fact, have the greatest capability for work in the middle period. Between child- hood and old age there is, however, this difference — that while in the former the non-available substances in the cell are developing, not yet having ma- tured, those in the latter have in some way become permanently useless. The degree to which the blood-supply can be controlled varies with age. and the amounts of substance capable of yielding energy at various periods of life are different; so that, considering these factors alone, though there are probably others, it may be easily appreciated that the sleep of childhood, maturity, and old age should be quite distinguishable. Cause of Sleep. — It is recognized that local exercise is capable of pro- ducing general fatigue, and the fatigued portions give rise to afferent impulses which, reaching the central system, cause some of the sensations of fatigue; moreover, the active tissue- (nerve-cells and muscles) yield as the result of their activity some by-product which is carried by the blood through the cen- tral system and becomes the chief cause of sleep. It has been shown by Mosso that if a dog be thoroughly fatigued, giving all the signs of exhaustion, and the blood from this dog be transfused to one that has been at rest, then after the transfusion, the dog which has received the blood from the exhausted animal will exhibit the symptoms of fatigue in full force. The inference is that from the tired animal certain by-products have thus been transferred, and that these are responsible for the reactions. We know, further, that we can distinguish in ourselves different forms of the feeling of fatigue, and that the sensations which follow the prolonged exercise of the muscular system differ from those following the exercise of the higher nerve-centres. Two things appear as highly probable: First, that there is a wide individual variation in the condition designated as normal sleep. Second, that normal sleep is the result of several sets of influences which need not necessarily be active to the same degree during each period of sleep. Excluding the factor represented by diminution of the external stimuli, sleep has been attributed more or less exclusively to one of the three following influences: 1. Chemical Influences. — The theories emphasizing the chemical factor point out that during the normal activity of the body there are formed and taken up by the blood substances which may directly diminish the activity of the nerve-cells and directly or reflexly affect the circulation so as to diminish the supply of blood to the brain, and especially to the cerebral cortex. •J. Circulatory Influences. — The vaso-motor theories look upon the changes in the blood-supply as :i prime cause of sleep; these changes to be referred in the last instance to the fatigue of the vaso-motor centre in the bulb. .'5. Histological Influences. — These are made dependent on the shrinkage of nerve-cells during fatigue, the retraction of the dendrites of the cortical cells interrupting the nerve-pathways, or the mechanical separation of the nerve-elements through the intrusion of the neuroglia-cells between them (Cajal). The vaso-motor and chemical theories combined are at present most CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. 293 worthy of attention, and Howell,1 after carefully reviewing the several theories of sleep, emphasizes the fatigue of the vaso-motor centre in the bulb as the important cause of the diminished blood-supply to the brain, this fatigue in turn being caused by the continuous activity of this centre during the waking- hours." Cessation of stimuli, decreased responsiveness of the active tissues, a change in the composition of the blood, and a diminution of the blood-supply to the brain are the preliminaries to sleep. A condition superficially resembling sleep can be induced in various ways. Removal of all external stimuli, extreme cold, anaesthetics, hypnotic sugges- tion, compression of the carotids, a blow on the head, loss of blood, all pro- duce a state of unconsciousness which, in so far, has the similitude of sleep. These conditions produce this state, however, by mechanically decreasing the blood-supply or cutting off the peripheral stimuli. 1 Mil. .i"»i ,1'n1"1' ii' il,i"|lil.l|,|",'l, "illininiii'lV"' n \ ., "' Fig. 123.— Plethysmography record taken from the arm of a person sleeping in the laboratory. A tail in the curve indicates a decrease in the volume of the arm. The curve is to be read in the direction of the arrow. 1. The night watchman entering the laboratory, waking the subject, who shortly fell a>leep again; 2, the watchman spoke; 3, watchman went out; these changes (2 and 3) occurred without awak- ening the subject (from experiments made by Messrs. Bardeen and Nichols, Johns Hopkins Medical School). Normal sleep is tested by the fact that during its progress the changes that occur in the central system are recuperative, whereas this feature may be almost absent in the states which nearly resemble it. Condition of the System During- Sleep. — It appears that during sleep the capacity of the central system to react is never lost. Were such the ease it would not be possible to awaken the sleeper. The reactions most depressed during sleep are those which require the lull activity of the cerebral cortex for their occurrence. Conversely, it is the spinal cord which is least affected. Moreover, the sleeping person is far more responsive to stimuli from without than at first might be thought, The close relations between dreams and ex- ternal stimuli have been recognized, and plethysmography studies show still more clearly how the matter stands. It was found that when a subject fell asleep with the arm in a plethys- mograph, various stimuli which did not waken the sleeper still served to cause a diminution in the volume of the arm which was certainly due to the 1 Howell : Journal of Experimental Medicine, 1897. 2 De Manaceine: "Sleep: Its Physiology, Pathology, Hygiene, and Psychology," Contem- porary Science Series, London, 1897. l".l 1 AN AMERICAN TEXT- BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. withdrawal of blood from it, the blood supplied to the brain being probably ;ii the same time increased (see Fig. 123). Tins experiment shows that during sleep the nervous system is capable of reactions which are not remembered in any way, but which naturally form a feature of the condition intermediate between full consciousness and deep slumber. The depth of sleep as determined by the strength of the stimulus necessary to elicit an efficient response has been measured. The stimulus in these ex- periments was the sound caused by the fall of a ball upon a plate, and the measure was the height from which the ball must fall in order to produce a sound loud enough to awaken a sleeping person. The results of the observa- tions are shown in Fig. 124. Strength of Stimulus 800 700 600 500 400 300 200 -A <> i 100 Hours 0.5 1.0 1.5 20 2.5 3.0 3.5 4.0 4.5 5.0 5.5 6.0 63 7.0 7.5 7.8 Pig. 124.— Curve illustrating the strength of an auditory stimulus (a ball falling from a height) neces- sary to waken a sleeping person. The hours marked below. The tests were made at half-hour intervals. The curve indicates that the distance through which the ball required to tie dropped increased during the first hour, and then diminished, at first very rapidly, then slowly (Kolsehiitter). It is seen from this that the period of deep slumber is short, less than two hours; and is followed by a long period, that of an average night's rest, dur- ing which a comparatively slight stimulus is sufficient to awaken. A some- what different curve has been more recently obtained by Monninghoff and Piesbergen.1 It is evident that the effectiveness of such a stimulus is, however, no measure of the recuperative processes in the central system. Repair is by no means accomplished during the interval of deep sleep, and experience has shown, as in the case of persons undertaking to walk a thousand miles in one thousand hours that although such an arrangement left the subject with two- 1 Zeitschrift fiir Biologic, 1893, Bd. xix. CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. 295 thirds of the total time for rest and refreshment, vet the feat was most difficult to accomplish by reason of the discontinuity in the sleep. The changes leading to recuperation needed longer periods than those permitted by the conditions of the experiment. Loss of Sleep. — Loss of sleep is more damaging to the organism as a whole than is starvation. It lias been found (Manace'ine) that in young dogs which can recover from starvation extending over twenty days, loss of sleep for five days or more was fatal. Toward the end of such a period the body- temperature may fall as much as 8° C. below the normal and the reflexes disappear. The red •blood-corpuscles are first diminished in number; to be finally increased during the last two days, when the animal refuses food. The most widespread change in the tissues is a tatty degeneration, and in the nervous system there were found capillary hemorrhages in the cerebral hemi- spheres, the spinal cord appearing abnormally dry and anaemic. Patrick and Gilbert l have studied the effects of loss of sleep in man (three subjects, young men, observed during ninety hours without sleep). All the subjects gained slightly in weight during the period, but lost this excess in the course of the first sleep following the experiment. The excre- tion of nitrogen and phosphoric acid was increased during the period, the increase being relatively greater in the case of the phosphoric acid. There was a marked tendency to a decrease in the pulse-rate, and some tendency for the body-temperature to fall. During these ninety hours the subjects were tested at intervals of six hours (the tests required some two hours on each occasion), to determine variations in the muscular and mental powers. In brief, it may be said that most tests revealed a loss, which early appeared in the reactions of the muscular system, and later in those of the nervous system. In the test for the acuteness of virion (measured by the distance at which the subject could read a printed page illuminated by the light of one standard candle at a distance of 25 cm.) there was, however, an increase in capability in all the subjects. At the end of the experiment a small number of hours of sleep in excess of that customarily taken appeared to bring about a complete restoration of the subject. The disproportion between this amount of extra sleep and the amount lost during the period of experiment is noted by the authors, though it still lacks satisfactory explanation. E. Old Age of the Central System. Metabolism in the Nerve-cells. — Connected closely with fatigue are those alterations both of the constituent nerve-cells and of the entire system found in old age. The picture of the changes in the living cells is that of anabolic and catabolic processes always going on, but varying in their absolute and relative intensity according to several conditions. Of these conditions one of the most important is the age of the individual. In youth and during the growing period of* life the anabolic changes appear within the daily cycle of activity and repose to overbalance the katabolic, the total expenditure of 1 Patrick and Gilbert : Psychological Review, 1896, vol. iii. No. 5. 296 AN AMERICAN TEXT- BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. energy increasing toward maturity. During middle life the two processes are more nearly in equilibrium, though the total expenditure of energy is probably greatest then ; and finally in old age the total expenditure of energy diminishes, while at the same time the anabolic processes become less and less competent to repair the waste. The question why in the nervous system the energies wane with advanced age is but the obverse of the question why they wax during the growing period. The essential nature of these changes is in both instances equally obscure. Decrease in Weight of the Brain. — Between the fiftieth and sixtieth years of life there is a decrease in the bulk of the encephalon in those persons belonging to the classes from which the greater number of the records have been obtained. So far as can be seen from the present records, there is no marked change in the proportional development of the encephalon in old age, though the loss appears to be slightly greater in the cerebral hemi- spheres than in the other portions. Changes in the Encephalon. — The thickness of the cerebral cortex diminishes in harmony with the shrinkage of the entire system, in large measure this must depend on the loss of volume in the various fibre-systems, which, according to the observations of Vulpius, show a senile decrease in the number of fibres composing them. This decrease is more marked in the motor than in the sensory areas. The time at which it commences cannot, however, be accurately stated, owing to the small number of records after the thirty-third year. Where records have been made between this and the seventy-ninth year it appears that there is no decided diminution until after the fiftieth year, though at the seventy-ninth year the decrease is clearly shown. Engel has shown that the branches of the arbor vita3 of the human cerebellum decrease in size and number in old age.1 Changes in the Cerebellum. — Tn the case of a man dying of old age (Hodge) some cells in the cerebellum were found shrunken and others (cells of Purkinje) had completely disappeared. In the antennary ganglion of bees a very striking difference appears between those dying of old age and the adult just emerged from its larval skin. These changes are comparable with those described in mammals, and it further appears that in passing from the youngesl to the oldest forms cells have disappeared from the ganglia and that in the young form of the bee there are some twenty-nine cells presenl for each one found at a later period. To the anatomy of the human nervous system in old age contributions have been made by studies on the pathological anatomy of paralysis agitans.2 In subjects suffering from this affection the bodies of the nerve-cells are shrunken, pigmented, and show in some cases a granular degeneration ; the fibres in part arc atrophied and degenerated ; the supporting tissues increase, and the walls of the small blood-vessels are thickened. These changes have been found principally in the spinal cord, being most marked in the lumbar ' Engel: Wiener medicinisehe Woehensehrift, 18f>3. 2 Ketch. -r : Zeitechrififur Heilkunde, 1892; Redlich: Jahrbueh fur Psychiatric, 1893. CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. 297 region. But the cords of aged persons who do not exhibit the symptoms of paralysis agitans show similar changes, though usually they are not so evident, and hence the pathological anatomy of this disease resolves itself into a somewhat premature and excessive senility of the central system. Shrinkage, decay, and destruction mark the progress of senescence, and the nervous system as a whole becomes less vigorous in its responses, less capable of repair or extra strain, and less permeable to the nervous impulses that fall upon it ; and it thus breaks down, not into the disconnected elements of the fetus, but into groups of elements, so that its capacities are lost in a fragmentary and uneven way. III. THE SPECIAL SENSES. A. Vision. The Physiology of Vision. — The eye is the organ by means of which certain vibrations of the lnminiferons ether are enabled to aifeet our conscious- ness, producing the sensation which we call " light." Hence the essential part of an organ of vision is a substance or an apparatus which, on the one hand, is of a nature to be stimulated by waves of light, and, on the other, is so con- nected with a nerve that its activity causes nerve-impulses to be transmitted to the nerve-centres. Any animal in which a portion of the ectoderm is thus differentiated and connected may be said to possess an eye — i. e. an organ through which the animal may consciously or unconsciously react to the exist- ence of light an Mind it.1 But the human eye, as well as that of all the higher animals, not only informs us of the existence of light, but enables us to form correct ideas of the direction from which the light comes and of the form, color, and distance of the luminous body. To accomplish this result the substance sensitive to light must form a part of a complicated piece of apparatus capable of very varied adjustments. The eye is, in other words, an optical instrument, and its description, like that of all optical instruments, includes a consideration of its mechanical adjustments and of its refracting media. Mechanical Movements. — The first point to be observed in studying the movements of the eye is that they are essentially those of a ball-and-socket joint, the globe of the eye revolving freely in the socket formed by the capsule of Tenon through a horizontal angle of almost 88° and a vertical angle of about 80°. The centre of rotation of the eye (which is not, however, an absolutely fixed point) does not coincide with the centre of the eyeball, but lies a little behind it. It is rather farther forward in hypermetropic than in myopic eyes. The movements of the eye, especially those in a horizontal direction, are sup- plemented by the movements of the head upon the shoulder-. The combined eye and head movements are in mosl persons sufficiently extensive to enable the individual, without any movement of the body, to receive upon the lateral portion of the retina the image of an object directly behind his back. The rotation of the eye in the socket is of course easiest and most extensive when the eyeball has an approximately spherical shape, as in the normal or emme- tropic eye. Winn the antero-posterior diameter is very much longer than those 1 In certain of the lower orders of animals no local differentiations seem to have occurred, and the whole surface of the body appears to be obscurely sensitive to light. See Nagel : Der lAchtxinn augenloser Thiere, Jena, 1896. 298 THE SENSE OF VISION. 299 at right angles to it, as in extremely myopic or short-sighted eyes, the rotation of the eyeball may be considerably limited in its extent. In addition to the movements of rotation round a centre situated in the axis of vision, the eye- ball may be moved forward and backward in the socket to the extent of about one millimeter. This movement may be observed whenever the eyelids are widely opened, and is supposed to be effected by the simultaneous contraction of both the oblique muscles. A slight lateral movement has also been described. The movements of the eye will be best understood when considered as referred to three axes at right angles to each other and passing through the centre of rotation of the eye. The first of these axes, which may be called the longitudinal axis, is best described as coinciding with the axis of vision when, with head erect, we look straight forward to the distant horizon ; the second, or transverse, axis is defined as a line passing through the centres of rotation of the two eyes; and the third, or vertical, axis is a vertical line nec- essarily perpendicular to the other two and also passing through the centre of rotation. When the axis of vision coincides with the longitudinal axis, the eye is said to be in the primary position. When it moves from the primary posi- tion by revolving around either the transverse or the vertical axis, it is said to assume secondary positions. All other positions are called tertiary positions, and are reached from the primary position by rotation round an axis which lies in the same plane as the vertical and horizontal axis — i. e. in the " equato- rial plane" of the eye. When the eye passes from a secondary to a tertia re- position, or from one tertiary position to another, the position assumed by the eye is identical with that which it would have had if it had reached it from the primary position by rotation round an axis in the equatorial plane. In other words, every direction of the axis of vision is associated with a fixed position of the whole eye — a condition of the greatest importance for the easy and correct use of the eyes. A rotation of the eye round its antero-posterior axis takes place in connection with certain movements, but authorities differ with regard to the direction and amount of this rotation. Muscles of the Eye. — The muscles of the eye are six in number — viz: the superior, inferior, internal and external recti, and the superior and inferior oblique. This apparent superfluity of muscles (for four muscles would suffice to turn the eye in any desired direction) is probably of advantage in reducing the amount of muscular exertion required to put the eye into any given posi- tion, and thus facilitating the recognition of slight differences of direction, for, according to Fechner's psycho-physic law the smallest perceptible difference in a sensation is proportionate to the total amount of the sensation. Hence if the eye can be brought into a given position by a slight muscular effort, a change from that position will be more easily perceived than if a powerful effort were necessary. Each of the eye-muscles, acting singly, tends to rotate the eye round an axis which may be called the axis of rotation of that muscle. Now, none of the muscles have axes of rotation King exactly in the equator of the eve — i. e. in a plane passing through the centre of rotation perpendicular to the axis 300 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. of vision.1 Bui all movements of the eye from the primary position take place, as we have seen, round an axis lying in this plane. Hence all such movements must be produced by more than one muscle, and this circumstance also is prob- ably of advantage in estimating the extent and direction of the movement. In this connection it is interesting to note that the eye-muscles have an exception- ally abundant nerve-supply — a fact which it is natural to associate with their power of extremely delicate adjustment. It has been found by actual count that in the muscles of the human eye each nerve-fibre supplies only two or three muscle-fibres, while in the muscles of the limbs the ratio is as high as 1 to 40-125.2 Although each eye has its own supply of muscles and nerves, yet the two eyes are not independent of each other in their movements. The nature of their connections with the nerve-centres is such that only those movements are, as a rule, possible in which both axes of vision remain in the same plane. This condition being fulfilled, the eyes may be together directed to any desired point above, below, or at either side of the observer. The axes may also be con- verged, as is indeed necessary in looking at near objects,- and to facilitate this convergence the internal recti muscles are inserted nearer to the cornea than the other muscles of the eye. Though in the ordinary use of the eyes there is never anv occasion to diverge the axes of vision, yet most persons are able to effect a divergence of about four degrees, as shown by their power to overcome the ten- dency to double vision produced by holding a prism in front of one of the eyes. The nervous mechanism through which this remarkable co-ordination of the muscles of the two eyes is effected, and their motions limited to those which are useful in binocular vision, is not completely understood, but it is supposed to have its seat in part in the tubercula quadrigemina, in connection with the nuclei of origin of the third, fourth, and sixth cranial nerves. Its disturbance by disease, alcoholic intoxication, etc. causes strabismus, confusion, dizziness, and double vision. A nerve termination sensitive to light, and so arranged that it can be turned in different directions, is sufficient to give information of the direction from which the light comes, for the contraction of the various eye-muscles indicates, through the nerves of muscular sense, the position into which the eye is nor- mally brought in order to best receive the luminous rays, or, in other words, tin; direction of the luminous body. The eye, however, informs us not only of the direction, but of the form of the object from which the light proceeds; and to understand how this is effected it will be necessary to consider the refracting media of the eve by means of which an optical image of the luminous object is thrown upon the expanded termination of the optic nerve — viz. the retina. Dioptric Apparatus of the Eye. — For the better comprehension of this portion of the subject a few definitions in elementary optics may be given. A 1 The axes of rotation of the internal and external recti, however, deviate but slightly from the equatorial plane. 2 1'. Tergast : '' Leber das Verbiiltniss von Nerven unci Muskeln," Archiv fixr mikr. Anat.. ix. 36-46. THE SENSE OF VISION. 301 dioptric system in its simplest form consists of two adjacent media which have different indices of refraction and whose surface of separation is the segment of a sphere. A line joining the middle of the segment with the centre of the sphere and prolonged in either direction is called the axis of the system. Let the line AP B in Figure 125 represent in section such a spherical surface the M'^ B Fig. 125.— Diagram of simple optical system (after Foster). centre of which is at N, the rarer medium being to the left and the denser me- dium to the right of the line. Any ray of light which, in passing from the rarer to the denser medium, is perpendicular to the spherical surface will be unchanged in its direction — i. e. will undergo no refraction. Such rays are represented by the lines OP, M I), and M' E. If a pencil of rays having its origin in the rarer medium at any point in the axis falls upon the spherical surface, there will be one ray — viz. the one which coincides with the axis of the system, which will pass into the second medium unchanged in its direction. This ray is called the principal ray (OP), and its point of intersection (P) with the spherical surface is called the principal point. The centre of the sphere (N) through which the principal ray necessarily passes is called the nodal point. All the other rays in the pencil are refracted toward the principal ray by an amount Fig. 126.— Diagram to show method of finding principal foci (Neumann). which depends, for a given radius of curvature, upon the difference in the refractive power of the media, or, in other words, upon the retardation of light in passing from one medium to the other. If the incident ray8 have their origin at a point infinitely distant on (he axis — i. e. if they are parallel to each other — they will all be refracted to a point behind the spherical surface known 302 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. as the principal focus, F. There is another 'principal focus (F) in front of the spherical surface — viz. the point from which diverging incident rays will be refracted into parallelism on passing the spherical surface, or, in other words, the point at which parallel rays coining from the opposite direction will be brought to a focus. The position of these two principal foci may be deter- mined bv the construction shown in Figure 126. Let CA C represent a sec- tion of a spherical refracting surface with the axis A N, the nodal point N, and the principal point .1. The problem is to find the foci of rays parallel to the axis. Erect perpendiculars at A and N. Set off on each perpendicular dis- tances No, Np, Ao', A]/ proportionate to the rapidity of light in the two media (e. ff. 2:3). The points where the lines p' o and po' prolonged will cut the axis are the two principal foci Fand F' — i. e. the points at which parallel rays coming from either direction are brought to a focus after passing the spherical refracting surface. If the rays are not parallel, but diverging — i. e. coming from an object at a finite distance — the point where the rays will be brought to a focus, or, in other words, the point where the optical image of the luminous object will be formed, may be determined by a construction which combines any two of the three rays whose course is given in the manner above described. Thus in Figure 1 27 let A N be the axis, and F and F' the principal foci of f"\ Fig. 127. — Diagram to show method of finding conjugate foci. the spherical refracting surface CA C, with a nodal poiut at N. Let B be the origin of a pencil of rays the focus of which is to be determined. Draw the line B C representing the course of an incident ray parallel to the axis. This rav will necessarily be refracted through the focus F, its course being represented by the line C F and its prolongation. Similarly, the incident ray passing through the focus F' and striking the spherical surface at C will, after refraction, be parallel to the axis — i. e. it will have the direction C b. The principal ray of the pencil will of course pass through the spherical surface and the nodal point N without change of direction. These three rays will come together at the same point b, the position of which may be determined by con- structing the course of any two of the three. The points B and b are called conjugate foci, and are related to each other in such a way that an optical image is formed at one point of a luminous object situated at the other. When the rays of light pass through several refracting surfaces in succession their course may be determined by separate calculations for each surface, a process which is much simplified when the surfaces are "centred" — i. e. have their centres of curvature lying in the same axis, as is approximately the case in the eye. Refracting- Media of the Eye. — Rays of light in passing through the eye penetrate seven different media and are retracted at seven surfaces. The media THE SENSE OF VISION. 303 are as follows : layer of tears, cornea, aqueous humor, anterior capsule of lens, lens, posterior capsule of lens, vitreous humor. The surfaces are those which separate the successive media from each other and that which separates the tear layer from the air. For purposes of practical calculation the number of sur- faces and media may be reduced to three. In the first place, the layer of tears which moistens the surface of the cornea has the same index of refraction as the aqueous humor. Hence the index of refraction of the cornea may be left out of account, since, having practically parallel surfaces and being bounded on both sides by substances having the same index of refraction, it does not influence the direction of rays of light passing through it. For this same reason objects seen obliquely through a window appear in their true direction, the refraction of the rays of light on entering the glass being equal in amount and opposite in direction to that which occurs in leaving it. For purposes of optical calculation we may, therefore, disregard the refraction of the cornea (which, moreover, does not differ materially from that of the aqueous humor), and imagine the aqueous humor extending forward to the anterior surface of the layer of tears which bathes the corneal epithelium. Furthermore, the cap- sule of the lens has the same index of refraction as the outer layer of the lens itself, and for optical purposes may be regarded as replaced by it. Hence the optical apparatus of the eye may be regarded as consisting of the fol- lowing three refracting media: Aqueous humor, index of refraction 1.33; lens, average index of refraction 1.45; vitreous humor, index of refraction 1.33. The surfaces at which refraction occurs are also three in number : An- terior surface of cornea, radius of curvature 8 millimeters; anterior surface of lens, radius of curvature 10 millimeters; posterior surface of lens, radius of curvature 6 millimeters. It will thus be seen that the anterior surface of the lens is less and the posterior surface more convex than the cornea. To the values of the optical constants of the eye as above given may be added the following : Distance from the anterior surface of the cornea to the anterior surface of the lens, 3.6 millimeters ; distance from the posterior sur- face of the lens to the retina, 15. millimeters ; thickness of lens, 3.<> millimeters. The methods usually employed for determining these constants are the fol- lowing: The indices of refraction of the aqueous and vitreous humor are determined by filling the space between a glass lens and a glass plate with the fresh humor. The aqueous or vitreous humor thus forms a convex or concave lens, from the form and focal distance of which the index can be calculated. Another method consists in placing a thin layer of the medium betweeu the hypothenuse surfaces of two right-angled prisms and determining the angle at which total internal reflection takes place. In the case of the crystalline lens the index is found by determining its focal distance as for an ordinary lens, and solving the equation which expresses the value of the index in terms of radius of curvature and focal distance, thickness, and focal length. The refractive index of the lens increases from the surface toward the centre, a peculiarity which tends to correct the disturbances due to spherical aberration, as well as to increase the refractive power of the lens ;is a whole. 304 AX AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. The curvature of the refracting surfaces of the eye is determined by an instrument known as an ophthalmometer, which measures the size of the reflected image of a known object in the various curved surfaces. The radius of curvature of the surface is determined by the following formula: B : b — A : - : or r = — , in which 11 = the size of the object, b = the size of 2 jB the image, A = distance between the object and the reflecting surface, and r = the radius of the reflecting surface. The distances between the various surfaces of the eye are measured on frozen sections of the organ, or can be determined upon the living eye by optical methods too complicated to be here described. It should be borne in mind that the above values of the so-called "optical constants" of the eye are subject to considerable individual variation, and that the statements of authors concerning them are not always consistent. The refracting surfaces of the eye may be regarded as still further sim- plified, and a so-called "reduced eye" constructed which is very useful for purposes of optical calculation. This reduced eye, which for optical purposes is the equivalent of the actual eye, is regarded as consisting of a single refract- ing medium having an index of 1.33, a radius of curvature of 5.017 milli- meters, its principal point 2.148 millimeters behind the anterior surface of the cornea, and its nodal point 0.04 millimeter in front of the posterior surface of the lens.1 The principal foci of the reduced eye are respectively 12.918 millimeters in front of and 22.231 millimeters behind the anterior surface of the cornea. Its optical power is equal to 50.8 dioptrics.2 The position of this imaginary refracting surface is indicated by the dotted line p in Figure 128. The Fig. 128— Diagram of the formation of a retinal image (after Foster). nodal point, n, in this construction may be regarded as the crossing-point of all the principal rays which enter tin.' eye, and, as these rays are unchanged in their direction by refraction, it is evident that the image of the point whence they proceed will be formed at the point where they strike the retina. Hence to determine the Bize and position of the retinal image of any external object — e. g. the arrow in Figure 128 — it is only necessary to draw lines from various 1 Strictly speaking, there are in thia imaginary refracting apparatus which is regarded as equivalent to the actual eye two principal and two nodal points, each pair about 0.4 millimeter apart. Tin- distance is so small that the two points may, for all ordinary constructions, be regarded as coincident. aThe optical power of a lens is the reciprocal of its focal length. The dioptry or unit of optical power is the power of a lens with a focal length of 1 meter. THE SENSE OE VISION. ;;o:, points of the object through the above-mentioned nodal point and to prolong them till they strike the retina. It is evident that the size of the retinal image will be as much smaller than that of the object as the distance of the nodal point from the retina is smaller than its distance from the object. According to the figures above given, the nodal point is about 7.2 milli- meters behind the anterior surface of the cornea and about 15.0 millimeters in front of the retina. Hence the size of the retinal image of an object of known size and distance can be readily calculated — a problem which has frequently to be solved in the study of physiological optics. The construction given in Figure 128 shows that from all external objects inverted images are projected upon the retina, and such inverted images can actually be seen under favorable condi- tions. If, for instance, the eye of a white rabbit, which contains no choroidal pigment, be excised and held with the cornea directed toward a window or other source of light, an inverted image of the luminous object will be seen through the transparent sclerotic in the same way that one sees an inverted image of a landscape on the ground-glass plate of a photographic camera. The question is often asked, " Why, if the images are inverted in the retina, do we not see objects upside down?" The only answer to such a question is that it is precisely because images are inverted on the retina that we do not see objects upside down, for we have learned through lifelong practice to asso- ciate an impression made upon any portion of the retina with light coming from the opposite portion of the field of vision. Thus if an image falls upon the lower portion of the retina, our experience, gained chiefly through mus- cular movements and tactile sensations, has taught us that this image must cor- respond to an object in the upper portion of our field of vision. In whatever way the retina is stimulated the same effect is produced. If, for instance, gentle pressure is made with the finger on the lateral portion of the eyeball through the closed lids a circle of light known as a phosphene immediately appears on the opposite side of the eye. Another good illustration of the same general rule is found in the effect of throwing a shadow upon the retina from an object as close as possible to the eye. For this purpose place a card B P Fig. 129.— Diagram Illustrating the projection of a shadow on the retina. with a small pin-hole in it in front of ;i source of light, and three or four centimeters distant from the eye — i. e. within the near point of distinct vision. Then hold some object smaller than the pupil — e.g. the head of a pin — as close as possible to the cornea. Under these conditions neither the pin-hole nor the pin-head can be really seen — i. e. they are Vol. II.— 20 306 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. both too near to have their images focussed upon the retina. The pin-hole becomes itself a source of light, and appears as a luminous circle bounded by the shadow thrown by the edge of the iris. Within this circle of light is seen the shadow of the pin-head, but the pin-head appears inverted, for the obvious reason that the eye, being accustomed to interpret all retinal impressions as corresponding to objects in the opposite portion of the field of vision, regards the upright shadow of the pin-head as the representation of an inverted object. The course of the rays in this experiment is shown in Figure 129, in which A B represents the card with a pin-hole in it, P the pin, and P' its upright shadow thrown on the retina. Accommodation. — From what has been said of conjugate foci and their relation to each other it is evident that any change in the distance of the object from the refracting media will involve a corresponding change in the position of the image, or, in other words, only objects at a given distance can be focussed upon a plane which has a fixed position with regard to the refracting surface or surfaces. Hence all optical instruments in which the principle of conjugate foci finds its application have adjustments for distance. In the telescope and opera-glass the adjustment is effected by changes in the distance between the lenses, and in the photographic camera by a change in the posi- tion of the ground-glass plate representing the focal plane. In the microscope the adjustment is effected by changing the distance of the object to suit the lenses, the higher powers having a shorter " working distance." We must now consider in what way the eye adapts itself to see objects dis- tinctly at different distances. That this power of adaptation, or " accommo- dation," really exists we can easily convince ourselves by looking at different objects through a network of fine wire held near the eyes. When with normal vision the eyes are directed to the distant objects the network nearly disappears, and if we attempt to see the network distinctly the outlines of the distant objects become obscure. In other words, it is impossible to see both the network and the distant objects distinctly at the same time. It is also evident that in accommodation for distant objects the eyes are at rest, for when they are suddenly opened after having been closed for a short time they are found to be accommodated for distant objects, and we are conscious of a distinct effort in directing them to any near object.1 From the optical principles above described it is clear that the accommo- dation of the eye for near objects may be conceived of as taking place in three different ways: 1st, By an increase of the distance between the refracting sur- faces of the eye and the retina; 2d, By an increase of the index of refraction of one or more of the media; 3d, By a diminution of the radius of curvature of one or more of the surfaces. The first of these methods was formerly sup- posed to be the one actually in use, a lengthening of the eyeball under a pres- 1 It has been shown by Beer {Archivjvsr die gesammte Phygiologie, lviii. 523) that in fishes the eyes when at rest are accommodated for neur objectB, and that accommodation for di.f the crystalline lens (Williams, after Donders). in the directions indicated by the dotted lines ending at a, b, and c. When the eye is accommodated for a near object the middle one of the three images moves nearer the corneal image — i. e. it changes in its direction from h to //, showing that the anterior surface of the lens has bulged forward into the position indi- THE SENSE OF VISION. 309 cated by the dotted line. The change in the appearance of the images is represented diagrammatically in Figure 132. On the left is shown the appear- ance of the images as seen when the eye is at rest, a representing the corneal image, b that reflected from the anterior, and c that from the posterior surface of the lens when the observing eye and the candle are in the position repre- Fig. 132.— Reflected images of a candle-flame as seen in the pupil of an eye at rest and accommodated for near objects (Williams). sented in Figure 131. The images are represented as they appear in the dark background of the pupil, though of course the corneal image may, in certain positions of the light, appear outside of the pupillary region. When the eye is accommodated for near objects the images appear as shown in the circle on the right, the image b becoming smaller and brighter and moving toward the corneal image, while the pupil contracts as indicated by the circle drawn round the images. The changes produced in the eye by an effort of accommodation are indi- cated in Figure 133, the left-hand side of the diagram showing the condition Fig. 133.— Showing changes in the eye produced by the act of accommodation (Helmholtz). of the eye at rest, and the right-hand side that in extreme accommodation for near objects. It will be observed that the iris is pushed forward by the bulging lens and that its free border approaches the median line. In other words, the pupil is contracted in accommodation for near objects. The following explanation of the mechanism by which this change in the shape of the lens is effected lias been proposed by Helmholtz, and is still generally accepted. The structure of the lens is such that by its own elasticity it tends constantly to assume a more convex form than tin1 pressure of the capsule and the tension of the sus- pensory ligaments (s, s, Fig. 133) allow. This pressure and tension are dimin- ished when the eye is accommodated for near vision by the contraction of the ciliary muscles (e, e, Fig. 133), most of whose fibres, having their origin at the 310 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. Fig. 134— To illustrate Scln ii-n's theory of ac- commodation. point of union of the cornea and sclerotic, extend radially outward in every direction and are attached to the front part of the choroid. The contrac- tion of the ciliary muscle, drawing forward the membranes of the eye, will relax the tension of the suspensory ligament and allow the lens to take the form determined by its own elastic structure. According to another theory of accommodation proposed by Tschcrning,1 the suspensory liga- ment is stretched and not relaxed by the contraction of the ciliary muscle. In consequence of the pressure thus produced upon the lens, the soft external portions are moulded upon the harder nuclear portion in such a way as to give to the anterior (and to some extent to the posterior) surface a hyperboloid instead of a spherical form. A similar theory has been recently brought forward by Schoen,2 who com- pares the action of the ciliary muscle upon the lens to that of the fingers compressing a rubber ball, as shown in Fig- ure 134. These theories have an advantage over that offered by Helmholtz, inasmuch as they afford a better explanation of the presence of circular fibres in the ciliary muscle. They also make the fact of so-called " astig- matic accommodation " comprehensible. This term is applied to the power said to be sometimes gradually acquired by persons with astigmatic3 eyes of correcting this defect of vision by accommodating the eye more strongly in one meridian than another. The theory of Tscherning is sup- ported by Crzellitzer ' as the result of investigations into the hyperboloid form of the lens in accommodation. On the other hand, it is maintained by Priestley Smith5 that this form of the lens is not inconsistent with the Helm- holtz theory. Moreover, it has been shown by Hess6 and Heine7 that in extreme accommodation the lens drops slightly toward the lower part of the eye, a movement which seems to indicate a relaxation of the suspensory liga- ment. The weight of evidence seems, therefore, on the whole, to be on the side of the theory of Helmholtz. Whatever views may be entertained as to the exact mechanism by which its change of shape is brought about, there can be no doubt that the lens is the portion of the eye chiefly or wholly concerned in accommodation, and it is accordingly found that the removal of the lens in the operation for cataract destroys the power of accommodation, and the patient is compelled to use convex lenses for distant and still stronger ones for near objects. It is interesting to notice that the act of accommodation, though distinctly voluntary, is performed by the agency of the wastriped fibres of the ciliary muscles. It is evident, therefore, that the term " involuntary " sometimes 1 Archives fie Physiologie, 1894, p. 40. 2 Archiv fur die gesammie Physiologie, lix. 427. ' flee |>. 317. * Archiv j'iir Ophthalmologic, xlii. (4) S. 36. 5 Ophthalmic Review, xvii. p. 341. 6 Archiv J'iir Ophthalmologic, xlii. S. 288, and xliii. S. 477. 7 Ibid., xliv. (2) S. 299, and xlvii. (2) S. 662. THE SENSE OF VISION. 311 applied to muscular fibres of this sort may be misleading. The voluntary character of the act of accommodation is not affected by the circumstance that the will needs, as a rule, to be assisted by visual sensations. The fact that most persons cannot affect the necessary change in the eye unless they direct their attention to some near or far object is only an instance of the close rela- tion between sensory impressions and motor impulses, which is further exem- plified by such phenomena as the paralysis of the lip of a horse caused by division of the fifth nerve. It is found, moreover, that by practice the power of accommodating the eye without directing it to near and distant objects can be acquired. The nerve-channels through which accommodation is affected are the anterior part of the nucleus of the third pair of nerves lying in the extreme hind part of the floor of the third ventricle, the most anterior bundle of the nerve-root, the third nerve itself, the lenticular ganglion, and the short ciliary nerves (see diagram p. 323). The mechanism of accommodation is affected in a remarkable way bv drugs, the most important of which are atropia and physostigmin, the former para- lyzing and the latter stimulating the ciliary muscle. As these drugs exert a corresponding effect upon the iris, it will be convenient to discuss their action in connection with the physiology of that organ. The changes occurring in the eye during the act of accommodation are indicated in the following table, which shows, both for the actual and the reduced eye, the extent to which the refracting media change their form and position, and the consequent changes in the position of the foci : Accommodation for Actual Eye. distant objects. near objects. Radius of cornea 8 mm. 8 mm. Radius of anterior surface of lens 10 6 " Radius of posterior surface of lens 6 " 5.5 " Distance from cornea to anterior surface of lens . . 3.6 " 3.2 " Distance from cornea to posterior surface of lens . 7.2 " 7.2 " Reduced Eye. Radius of curvature 5.02 " 4.48 " Distance from cornea to principal point 2.15 " 2.26 " Distance from cornea to nodal point 7.16 " 6.74 " Distance from cornea to anterior focus 12.918 " 11.241 " Distance from cornea to posterior focus 22.231 " 20. 248 " It will be noticed that no change occurs in the curvature of the cornea, and next to none in the posterior surface of the lens, while the anterior surface of the lens undergoes material alterations both in its shape and position. Associated with the accommodative movements above described, two other changes take place in the eyes to adapt them for near vision. In the first place, the axes of the eyes are converged upon the near object, so that the images formed in the two eyes shall fall upon corresponding points of* the retinas, as will be more fully explained in connection with the subject of binocular vision. In the second place, the pupil becomes contracted, thus reducing the size of the pencil of rays that enters the eye. The importance of this movement of the pupil will be belter understood after the subject of 312 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OE PHYSIOLOGY. spherical aberration of light has been explained. These three adjustments, focal, axial, and pupillary, are so habitually associated in looking at near objects that the axial can only by an effort be dissociated from the other two, while these two are quite inseparable from one another. This may be illustrated by a simple experiment. On a sheet of paper about 40 centimeters distant from the eyes draw two letters or figures precisely alike and about 3 centimeters apart. (Two letters cut from a newspaper and fastened to the sheet will answer the same purpose.) Hold a small object like the head of a pin between the eyes and the paper at the point of intersection of a line joining the right eye and the left letter with a line joining the left eye and the right letter. If the axes of vision are converged upon the pin-head, that object will be seen dis- tinctly, and beyond it will be seen indistinctly three images of the letter, the central one being formed by the blending of the inner one of each pair of images formed on the two retinas. If now the attention be directed to the middle image, it will gradually become perfectly distinct as the eye accommo- dates itself for that distance. We have thus an axial adjustment for a very near object and a focal adjustment for a more distant one. If the pupil of the individual making this observation be watched by another person, it will be found that at the moment when the middle image of the letter becomes distinct the pupil, which had been contracted in viewing the pin-head, suddenly dilates. It is thus seen that when the axial and focal adjustments are dissociated from each other the pupillary adjustment allies itself with the latter. The opposite form of dissociation — viz. the axial adjustment for distance and the focal adjustment for near vision — is less easy to bring about. It may perhaps be best accomplished by holding a pair of stereoscopic pictures before the eyes and endeavoring to direct the right eye to the right and the left eye to the left picture — i. e. to keep the axes of vision parallel while the eyes are accommodated for near objects. One who is successful in this species of ocular gymnastics sees the two pictures blend into one having all the appearance of a solid object. The power of thus studying stereoscopic pictures without a stereoscope is often a great convenience to the possessor, but individuals differ very much in their ability to acquire it. Range of Accommodation. — By means of the mechanism above described it is possible for the eye to produce a distinct image upon the retina of objects lying at various distances from the cornea. The point farthest from the eye at which an object can be distinctly seen is called the far-point, and the nearest point of distinct vision is called the near-point of the eye, and the distance between the near-point and the far-point is called the range of distinct vision or the range of accommodation. As the normal emmetropic eye is adapted, when at rest, to bring parallel rays of light to a focus upon the retina, its far- point may be regarded as at an infinite distance. Its near-point varies with age, as will be described under Presbyopia. In early adult life it is from 10 to 13 centimeters from the eye. For every point within this range there will be theoretically a corresponding condition of the lens adapted to bring rays pro- ceeding from that point to a focus on the retina, but as rays reaching the eye from a point 175 to 200 centimeters distant do not, owing to the small size of THE SENSE OF VISION. 313 the pupil, differ sensibly from parallel rays, there is no appreciable change in the lens unless the object looked at lies within that distance. It is also evi- dent that as an object approaches the eye a given change of distance will cause a constantly increasing amount of divergence of the rays proceeding from it, and will therefore necessitate a constantly increasing amount of change in the lens to enable it to focus the rays on the retina. We find, accordingly, that all objects more than two meters distant from the eye can be seen distinctly at the same time — i. e. without any change in the accommodative mechanism — but for objects within that distance we are conscious of a special effort of accommodation which becomes more and more distinct the shorter the distance between the eye and the object. Myopia and Hypermetropia. — There are two conditions of the eye in which the range of accommodation may differ from that which has just been described as normal. These conditions, which are too frequent to be regarded (except in extreme cases) as pathological, are generally dependent upon the eyeball being unduly lengthened or shortened. In Fig. 135 are shown diagram matically the three conditions known as emmetropia, myopia, and hypermetropia. In the normal or emmetropic eye, A, parallel rays are represented as brought to a focus on the retina ; in the short-sighted, or myopic, eye, B, similar rays are focussed in front of the retina, since the latter is abnormally distant; while in the over-sighted, or hypermetropic, eye, C, they are focussed behind the retina, since it is abnormally near. It is evident that when the eye is at rest both the myopic and the hy- permetropic eye will see distant ob- jects indistinctly, but then; is this important difference : that in hyper- metropia the difficulty can be cor- rected by an effort of accommodation, while in myopia this is impossible, since there is no mechanism by which the radius of the lenticular surfaces can be increased. Hence an individual attected with myopia is always aware of the infirmity, while a person with hypermetropic eyes often goes through life unconscious of the defect. In this case the accomodation is constantly called into play even for distant objects, and if the hypermetropia is excessive, any prolonged use of the eyes is apt to be attended by a feeling of fatigue, headache, and a train of nervous symptoms familiar to the ophthalmic surgeon. Hence it i- important to discover this delict where it exists and to apply the appropriate remedy — viz. convex lenses placed Fig. 135.— Diagram showing the difference between normal, myopic, aud hypermetropic eyes, 314 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. in front of the eyes in order to make the rays slightly convergent when they enter the eye. 'finis aided, the refractive power of the eye at rest is sufficient to bring the rays to a focus upon the retina and thus relieve the accommoda- tion. This action of a convex lens in hypermetropia is indicated by the dotted lines in Fig. 135, C, and the corresponding use of a concave lens in myopia is shown in Fig. 135, B. The detection and quantitative determination of hypermetropia are best made after the accommodation has been paralyzed by the use of atropia, by ascertaining how strong a convex lens must be placed before the eye to pro- duce distinct vision of distant objects. The range of accommodation varies very much from the normal in myopic and hypermetropic eyes. In myopia the near-point is often 5 or 6 centimeters from the cornea, while the far-point, instead of being infinitely far off, is at a variable but no very great distance from the eye. The range of accommoda- tion is therefore very limited. In hypermetropia the near-point is slightly farther than normal from the eye, and the far-point cannot be said to exist, for the eye at rest is adapted to bring converging rays to a focus on the retina, and such pencils of rays do not exist in nature. Mathematically, the far-point may be said to be at more than an infinite distance from the eye. The range of effective accommodation is therefore reduced, for a portion of the accommo- dative power is used up in adapting the eye to receive parallel rays. Presbyopia. — The power of accommodation diminishes with age, owing apparently to a loss of elasticity of the lens. The change is regularly pro- gressive, and can be detected as early as the fifteenth year, though in normal eyes it does not usually attract attention until the individual is between forty and forty-five years of age. At this period of life a difficulty is commonly experienced in reading ordinary type held at a convenient distance from the eye, and the individual becomes old-sighted or presbyopic — a condition which can, of course, be remedied by the use of convex glasses. According to Helmholtz, the far-point also recedes somewhat after fifty years of age. Hence emmetropic eyes may become hypermetropic and slightly myopic eyes emmetropic. Cases are occasionally reported of persons recovering their power of near vision in extreme old age and discontinuing the use of the glasses previously employed for reading. In these cases there is apparently not a restoration of the power of accommodation, but an increase in the refrac- tive power of the lens through local changes in its tissue. A diminution in the size of the pupil, sometimes noticed in old age,1 may also contribute to the distinctness of the retinal image, as will be described in connection with spherical aberration. Defects of the Dioptric Apparatus. — The above-described imperfections of the eye — viz. myopia and hypermetropia — being generally (though not invariably) due to an abnormal length of the longitudinal axis, are to be regarded as defects of construction affecting only a comparatively small 1 The average diameter of the pupil is said to be in youth 4.1 mm. and in old age 3 mm. Silberkuhl: Archiv/ur Ophthalmologic, xlii. (3) S. 179. THE SENSE OF VISION. 315 number of eyes. There are, however, a number of imperfections of the diop- tric apparatus, many of which affect all eyes alike. Of these imperfections some affect the eye in common with all optical instruments, while others are peculiar to the eye and are not found in instruments of human construction. The former class will be first considered. Spherical Aberration. — It has been stated that a pencil of rays falling upon a spherical refracting surface will be refracted to a common focus. Strictly speaking, however, the outer rays of the pencil — i. e. those which fall near the periphery of the refracting surface — will be refracted more than those which lie near the axis and will come to a focus sooner. This phenomenon, which is called spherical aberration, is more marked with diverging than with parallel rays, and tends, of course, to produce an indistinctness of the image which will increase with the extent of the surface through which the rays pass. The effect of a diaphragm used in many optical instruments to reduce the amount of spherical aberration by cutting off the side rays is shown dia- grammatically in Fig. 136. Fig. 136.— Diagram showing the effect of a diaphragm in reducing the amount of spherical aberration. The role of the iris in the vision of near objects is now evident, for when the eye is directed to a near object the spherical aberration is increased in con- sequence of the rays becoming more divergent, but the contraction of the pupil which accompanies accommodation tends, by cutting off the side rays, to prevent a blurring of the image which otherwise would be produced. It must, however, be remembered that the crystalline lens, unlike any lens of human construction, has a greater index of refraction at the centre than at the periph- ery. This, of course, tends to correct spherical aberration, and, in so far as it does so, to render the cutting off of the side rays unnecessary. Indeed, the total amount of possible spherical aberration in the eye is so small that its effect on vision maybe regarded :is insignificant in comparison with that caused by the other optical imperfections of the eye. 316 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. Chromatic Aberration. — In the above account of the dioptric apparatus of the eye the phenomena have been described as they would occur with mono- chromatic light — i. c with light having but one degree of refrangibility. But the light of the sun is composed of an infinite number of rays of different degrees of refrangibility. Hence when an image is formed by a simple lens the more refrangible rays — i. c. the violet rays of the spectrum — are brought to a focus sooner than the less refrangible red rays. The image therefore appears bordered by fringes of colored light. This phenomenon of chromatic aberration can be well observed by looking at objects through the lateral por- tion of a simple lens, or, still better, by observing them through two simple lenses held at a distance apart equal to the sum of their focal distances. The objects will appear inverted (as through an astronomical telescope) and sur- rounded with borders of colored light. Now, the chromatic aberration of the eve is so slight that it is not easily detected, and the physicists of the eighteenth century, in their efforts to produce an achromatic lens, seem to have been impressed by the fact that in the eye a combination of media of different refractive powers is employed, and to have sought in this circumstance an explanation of the supposed achromatism of the eye. Work directed on this line was crowned with brilliant success, for by combining two sorts of glass of different refractive and dispersive powers it was found possible to refract a ray of light without dispersing it into its different colored rays, and the achromatic lens, thus constructed, became at once an essential part of every first-class opti- cal instrument. Now, as there is not only no evidence that the principle of the achromatic lens is employed in the eye, but distinct evidence that the eye is uncorrected for chromatic aberration, we have here a remarkable instance of a misconception of a physical fact leading to an important discovery in physics. The chromatic aberration of the eye, though so slight as not to interfere at all with ordinary vision, can be readily shown to exist by the simple experiment of covering up one half of the pupil and, looking at a bright source of light e. g. a window. If the lower half of the pupil be covered, the cross-bars of Fig. 137.— Diagram to illustrate chromatic aberration. the window will appear bordered with a fringe of blue light on the lower and reddish light on the upper side. The explanation usually given of the way in which tin- result is produced is illustrated in Fig. 137. Owing to the chromatic aberration of the eye all the rays emanating from an object at A are not focussed accurately on the retina, but if the eye is accommodated for a ray of medium refrangibility, the violet rays will be brought to a focus in front of the retina at V, while the red rays will be focussed behind the retina at R. THE SENSE OE VISION. 317 On the retina itself will be formed not an accurate optical image of the point A, but a small circle of dispersion in which the various colored rays are mixed together, the violet rays after crossing falling upon the same part of the retina as the red rays before crossing. Thus by a sort of compensation, which, how- ever, cannot be equivalent to the synthetic reproduction of white light by the union of the spectral colors, the disturbing effect of chromatic aberration is diminished. When the lower half of the pupil is covered by the edge of a card held in front of the cornea at D, the aberration produced in the upper half of the eye is not compensated by that of the lower half. Hence the image of a point of white light at A will appear as a row of spectral colors on the retina, and all objects will appear bordered by colored fringes. Another good illustration of the chromatic aberration of the eye is obtained by cutting two holes of any convenient shape in a piece of black cardboard and placing behind one of them a piece of blue and behind the other a piece of red glass. If the card is placed in a window some distance (10 meters) from the observer, in such a position that the white light of the sky may be seen through the col- ored glasses, it will be found that the outlines of the two holes will generally be seen with unequal distinctness. To most eyes the red outline will appear quite distinct, while the blue figure will seem much blurred. To a few indi- viduals the blue figure appears the more distinct, and these will generally be found to be hypermetropic. Fig. 138.— Model to illustrate astigmatism. Astigmatism. — The defect known as astigmatism is due to irregularities of curvature of the refracting surfaces, in consequence of which all the rays proceeding from a single point cannot be brought to a single focus on the retina. Astigmatism is said to be regular when one of the surfaces, generally the 318 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. cornea, is not spherical, but ellipsoidal — i. e. having meridians of maximum and minimum curvature at right angles to each other, though in each meridian the curvature is regular. When this is the ease the rays proceeding from a single luminous point arc brought to a focus earliest when they lie in the meridian in which the surface is most convex. Hence the pencil of rays will have two linear foci, at right angles to the meridians of greatest and least curvature separated by a space in which a section of the cone of rays will be first elliptical, then circular, and then again elliptical. This defect exists to a certain extent in nearly all eyes, and is, in some cases, a serious obstacle to dis- tinct vision. The course of the rays when thus refracted is illustrated in Fig. 138, which represents the interior of a box through which black threads are drawn to indicate the course of the rays of light. The threads start at one end of the box from a circle representing the cornea, and converge with different degrees of rapidity in different meridians, so that a section of the cone of rays will be successively an ellipse, a straight line, an ellipse, a circle, etc., as shown by the model represented in Fig. 139. It will be noticed that this and the preced- Fig. 139.— Model to illustrate astigmatism. ing figure are drawn in duplicate, but that the lines are not precisely alike on the two sides. In fact, the lines on the left represent the model as it would be seen with the right eye, and those on the right' as it would appear to the left eye, which is just the opposite from an ordinary stereoscopic slide. The figures are drawn in this way because they are intended to produce a " pseudoscopic " effect in a way which will be explained in connection with the subject of binocular vision. For this purpose it is only necessary to cross the axes of vision in front of the page, as in the experiment described on page 312, for studying the relation between the focal, axial, and pupillary adjust- ments of the eye. As soon as the middle image becomes distinct it assumes a THE SENSE OF VISION. 319 stereoscopic appearance, and the correct relations between the different parts of the model are at once obvious. This imperfection of the eye may be detected by looking at lines such as are shown in Figure 140, and testing each eye separately. If the straight lines drawn in various directions through a common point cannot be seen with equal distinctness at the same time, it is evident that the eye is better adapted to focus rays in one meridian than in another — i. e. it is astigmatic. The concentric Fig. 140.— Lines for the detection of astigmatism. circles are a still more delicate test. Few persons can look at this figure attentively without noticing that the lines are not everywhere equally distinct, but that in certain sectors the circles present a blurred appearance. Not infrequently it will be found that the blurred sectors do not occupy a constant position, but oscillate rapidly from one part of the series of circles to another. This phe- nomenon seems to be due to slight involuntary contractions of the ciliary muscle causing changes in accommodation. The direction of the meridians of greatest and least curvature of the cornea of a regularly astigmatic eye, and the difference in the amount of this curvature, can be very accurately measured by means of the ophthalmometer (see p. 304). These points being determined, the defect of the eye can be perfectly corrected by cylindrical glasses adapted to compensate for the excessive or deficient refraction of the eye in certain meridians. By another method known as " skiascopy," which consists in studying the light reflected from the fundus of the eye when the ophthalmoscopic minor is moved in various directions, the amount and direction of the astigmatism of the eye as a whole (and not that of the cornea alone) may be ascertained. Astigmatism is said to be irregular when in certain meridians the curvatures of the refracting surfaces are not arcs of circles or ellipses, or when there is a lack of homogeneousness in the refracting media. This imperfection exists to a greater or less extent in all eves, and, unlike regular astigmatism, is incapable of correction. It manifests itself by causing the outlines of* all brilliant objects to appear irregular. It is on this account that the fixed stars do not appear to us like points of light, but as luminous bodies with irregular " star "-shaped outlines. The phenomenon can be conveniently studied by looking at a pin- hole in a large black card held at a convenient distance between the eve and a strong light. The hole will appear to have an irregular outline, and to some eyes will appear double or treble. 320 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. Intraocular Images. — Light entering the eye makes visible, under certain circumstances, a number of objects which lie within the eye itself. These objects are usually opacities in the media of the eye which are ordinarily invisi- ble, because the retina is illuminated by light coming from all parts of the pupil, and with such a broad source of light no object, unless it is a very large one or one lying very near the back of the eye, can cast a shadow on the retina. Such shadows can, however, be made apparent by allowing the media of the eye to be traversed by parallel rays of light. This can be accomplished by holding a small polished sphere — e. g. the steel head of a shawl-pin illuminated by sunlight or strong artificial light — in the anterior focus of the eye — i. e. about 22 millimeters in front of the cornea, or by placing a dark screen with a pin-hole in it in the same position between the eye and a source of uniform diffused light, such as the sky or the porcelain shade of a student lamp. In either case the rays of light diverging from the minute source will be refracted into parallelism by the media of the eye, and will produce the sensation of a circle of diffused light, the size of which will depend upon the amount of dila- tation of the pupil. Within this circle of light will be seen the shadows of any opaque substances that may be present in the media of the eye. These shadows, being cast by parallel rays, will be of the same size as the objects themselves, as is shown diagrammatically in Figure 141, in which A represents a source Fig. 141.— Showing the method of studying intraocular images (Helmholtz). of light at the anterior focus of the eye, and o an opacity in the vitreous humor casting a shadow B of the same size as itself upon the retina. It is evident that if the source of light A is moved from side to side the various opacities will be displaced relatively to the circle of light surrounding them by an amount de- pending upon the distance of the opacities from the retina. A study of these displacements will therefore afford a means of determining the position of the opacities within the media of the eye. Muscae Volitantes. — Among the objects to be seen in thus examining the eye the most conspicuous are those known as the muscce volitantes. These pre- sent themselves in the form of beads, either singly or in groups, or of streaks, patches, and granules. They have an almost constant floating motion, which is increased by the movements of the eye and head. They usually avoid the line of vision, floating away when an attempt is made to fix the sight upon them. When the eye is directed vertically, however, they sometimes place themselves directly in line with the object looked at. If the intraocular object is at the same time sufficiently near the back of the eye to cast a shadow which THE SENSE OE VISION. 321 is visible without the use of the focal illumination, some inconvenience may thus be caused in using a vertical microscope. A study of the motions of the m/uscce volitantes makes it evident that the phenomenon is due to small bodies floating in a liquid medium of a little greater specific gravity than themselves. Their movements are chiefly in planes perpendicular to the axis of vision, for when the eye is directed verti- cally upward they move as usual through the field of vision without increasing the distance from the retina. They are generally supposed to be the remains of the embyronic structure of the vitreous body — i. e. portions of the cells and fibres which have not undergone complete mucous transformation. In addition to these floating opacities in the vitreous body various other defects in the transparent media of the eye may be revealed by the method of focal illumination. Among these may be mentioned spots and stripes due to irregularities in the lens or its capsule, and radiating lines indicating the stel- late structure of the lens. Retinal Vessels. — Owing to the fact that the blood-vessels ramify near the anterior surface of the retina, while those structures which are sensitive to light constitute the posterior layer of that organ, it is evident that light entering the eye will cast a shadow of the vessels on the light-perceiving elements of the retina. Since, however, the diameter of the largest blood-vessels is not more than one-sixth of the thickness of the retina, and the diameter of the pupil is one-fourth or one-fifth of the distance from the iris to the retina, it is evident that when the eye is directed to the sky or other broad illuminated surfaces it is only the penumbra of the vessels that will reach the rods and cones, the umbra terminating conically somewhere in the thickness of the retina. But if light is allowed to enter the eye through a pin-hole in a card held a short distance from the cornea, as in the above-described method of focal illumination, a sharply defined shadow of the vessels will be thrown on the rods and cones. Yet under these conditions the retinal vessels are not rendered visible unless the perforated card is moved rapidly to and fro, so as to throw the shadow continually on to fresh portions of the retinal surface. When this is done the vessels appear, ramifying usually as dark lines on a lighter background, but the dark lines are sometimes bordered by bright edges. It will be observed that those vessels appear most distinctly the course of which is at right angles to the direction in which the card is moved. Hence in order to see all the vessels with equal distinctness it is best to move the card rapidly in a circle the diameter of which should not exceed that of the pupil. In this manner the distribution of the vessels in one's own retina may be accurately observed, and in many cases the position of the fovea centralis may be determined l>\ the absence of vessels from that portion of the macula lutea. The retinal vessels may also be made visible in several other ways — e. AX AMKIUCAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. described on p. 321. Another argument in favor of this view is found in the correspondence between the size of the smallest visible images on the retina and the diameter of the rods and cones. A double star can be recognized as double by the normal eye when the distance between the components corresponds to a visual angle of 60". Two white lines on a black ground are seen to be dis- tinct when the distance between them subtends a visual angle of 64"-73". These angles correspond to a retinal image of 0.0044, 0.0046, and 0.0053 mil- limeter. Now, the diameter of the cones in the macula lutea, as determined by Kolliker, is 0.0045-0.0055 millimeter, a size which agrees well with the hypothesis that each cone when stimulated can produce a special sensation of light distinguishable from those caused by the stimulation of the neighboring cones. The existence of the so-called blind spot in the retina at the point of entrance of the optic nerve is sometimes regarded as evidence of the light- perceiving function of the rods and cones, but as the other layers of the retina, as well as the rods and cones, are absent at this point, and the retina here consists solely of nerve-fibres, it is evident that the presence of the blind spot Fig. 145.— To demonstrate the blind spot. only proves that the optic nerve-fibres are insensible to light. Figure 145 is intended to demonstrate this insensibility. For this purpose it should be held at a distance of about 23 centimeters from the eyes (i. e. about 3.5 times the dis- tance between the cross and the round spot). If the left eye be closed and the right eye fixed upon the cross, the round spot will disappear from view, though it will become visible if the eye be directed either to the right or to the left of the cross, or if the figure be held either a greater or a less distance from the eye. The size and shape of the blind spot may readily be determined as follows : Fix the eye upon a definite point marked upon a sheet of white paper. Bring the black point of a lead pencil (which, except the point, has been painted white or covered with white paper) into the invisible portion of the field of vision and carry it outward in any direction until it becomes vis- ible. Mark upon the paper the point at which it just begins to be seen, and by repeating the process in as many different directions as possible the out- line of the blind spot may be marked out. Figure 146 shows the shape of the blind spot determined by Helm- holtz in his own right eye, a being A*Z Z TTTTZ ,,, , . ,. / the point of fixation of the eye, and Fig. 146.— Form of the blind spot (Ilulmholtz). ' J ' the line A B being one-third of the distance between the eye and the paper. The irregularities of outline, as at THE SENSE OF VISION. 329 Rods. Cones. d, are due to shadows of the large retinal vessels. During this determination it is of course necessary that the head should occupy a fixed position with regard to the paper. This condition can be secured by holding firmly between the teeth a piece of wood that is clamped in a suitable position to the edge of the table. The diameter of the blind spot, as thus determined, has been found to correspond to a visual angle varying from 3° 39' to 9° 47', the average measurement being 6° 10'. This is about the angle that is subtended by the human face seen at a distance of two meters. Although a considerable por- tion of the retina is thus insensible to light, we are, in the ordinary use of the eyes, conscious of no corresponding blank in the field of vision. By what psychical operation we " fill up " the gap in our subjective field of vision caused by the blind spot of the retina is a question that has been much dis- cussed without being definitely settled. The above-mentioned reasons for regarding the rods and cones as the light- perceiving elements of the retina seem sufficiently conclusive. Whether there is any difference between the rods and the cones with regard to their light- perceiving function is a question which may be best considered in connection with a description of the qualitative modifications of light. The histological relation between the various layers of the retina is still under discussion. According to recent observations of Cajal,1 the connection between the rods and cones on the one side and the fibres of the optic nerve on the other is established in a man- ner which is represented diagram- matical ly in Figure 147. The pro- longations of the bipolar cells of the internal nuclear layer E break up into fine fibres in the external molecular (or plexiform) layer C. Here they are brought into contact, though not into anatomical continuity, with the termi- nal fibres of the rods and cones. The inner prolongations of the same bipolar cells penetrate into the internal molec- ular (or plexiform) layer F, and there come into contact with the dendrites coming from the layer of ganglion-cells G. These cells are, in their turn, con- nected by their axis-cylinder processes with the fibres of the optic nerve. The bipolar cells which serve as connective links between the rods and the optic nerve-fibres are anatomically distin- guishable (as indicated in the diagram) i [i 147. — Diagrammatic representation of the structure of the retina (Cajal): .i, layer of rods and cones ; u, external nuclear layer ; ( . external molecular (or plexiform) layer; /•.', Internal nu- clear layer; F, Internal molecular (or plexiform) layer: 3. Saturation, dependent upon the amount of white light mingled with the monochromatic light. These three qualitative modifications of light must now be considered in detail. Color. — In our profound ignorance of the nature of the process by which, in the rods and cones, the movements of the ether waves are converted into a stimulus for the optic nerve-fibres, all that can be reasonably demanded of a color theory is that it shall present a logically consistent hypothesis to account for the sensations actually produced by the impact of ether waves of varying rates, either singly or combined, upon different parts of the retina. Some of the important phenomena of color sensation of which every color theory must take account may be enumerated as follows : 1. Luminosity is more readily recognized than color. This is shown by the fact that a colored object appears colorless when it is too feebly illuminated, and that a spectrum produced by a very feeble light shows variations of inten- sity with a maximum nearer than normal to the blue end, but no gradations of color. A similar lack of color is noticed when a colored object is observed for too short a time or when it is of insufficient size. In all these respects the various colors present important individual differences which will be con- sidered later. 2. Colored objects seen with increasing intensity of illumination appear more and more colorless, and finally present the appearance of pure white. Yellow passes into white more readily than the other colors. 3. The power of the retina to distinguish colors diminishes from the cent re toward the periphery, the various colors, in this respect also, differing mate- rially from each other. Sensibility to red is lost at a short distance from the macula lutea, while the sensation of blue is lost only on the extreme lateral portions of the retina. The relation of this phenomenon to the distribution of the rods and cones in the retina will be considered in connection with the perception of the intensity of light. Color-mixture. — Since the various spectral colors are produced by the dis- persion of the white light of the sun, it is evident that white light may be reproduced by the reunion of the rays corresponding to the different colors, and it is accordingly found that if the colored rays emerging from :i prism, :i» in Fig. 149, are reunited by suitable refracting surfaces, a spot of white light will be produced similar to that which would have been caused by the original beam of sunlight. But white light may be produced not only by the union of "// the spectral colors, but by the union of certain selected colors in twos, threes, 334 AN AMERICAN TEXT- BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. fours, etc. Any two spectral colors which by their union produce white are said to be " complementary " colors. The relation of these pairs of comple- mentary colors to each other may be best understood by reference to Figure 150. p Fig. 150.— Color diagram. Here the spectral colors are supposed to be disposed around a curved line, as indicated by their initial letters, and the two ends of the curve are united by a straight line, thus enclosing a surface having somewhat the form of a tri- angle with a rounded apex. If the curved edge of this surface be supposed to be loaded with weights proportionate to the luminosity of the different colors, the centre of gravity of the surface will be near the point W. Now, if a straight line be drawn from any point on the curved line through the point W and prolonged till it cuts the curve again, the colors corresponding to the two ends of this straight line will be complementary colors. Thus in Figure 150 it will be seen that the complementary color of red is bluish-green, and that of yellow lies near the indigo. It is also evident that the complementary color of green is purple, which is not a spectral color at all, but a color obtained by the union of violet and red. The union of a pair of colors King nearer together than complementary colors produces an intermediate color mixed with an amount of white which is proportionate to the nearness of the colors to the complementary. Thus the union of red and yellow produces orange, but a less saturated orange than the spectral color. The union of two colors lying farther apart than complementary colors produces a color which borders more or less upon purple. The mixing of colors to demonstrate the above-mentioned effects may be accomplished in three different ways: 1. Bv employing two prisms to produce two independent spectra, and then directing the colored rays which are to be united so that they will illuminate the same white surface. 2. By looking obliquely through a glass plate at a colored object placed behind it, while at the same time light from another colored object, placed in trout of the glass, is reflected into the eye of the observer, as shown in Figure 151. Here the transmitted light from the colored object A and the reflected light from the colored object B enter the eye at C from the same direction, ami are therefore united upon the retina. 3. Bv rotating before the eye a disk on which the colors to be united are painted upon different sectors. This is most readily accomplished by using THE SENSE OF VISION. 335 a number of disks, each painted with one of the colors to be experimented with, and each divided radially by a cut running from the centre to the circum- ference. The disks can then be lapped over each other and rotated together, and in this way two or more colors can be mixed in any desired proportions. This method of mixing colors depends upon the property of the retina to retain an impression after the stimulus causing /' it has ceased to act — a phenomenon of / great importance in physiological optics, .' \ and one which will be further discussed / U \ / \ in connection with the subject of " after- / \ / \ images." A/ \B The physiological mixing of Colors Fig. 151 — Diagram to illustrate color mixture by ,i |« v. j -i .1 • , reflected and transmitted light (Helmholtz). cannot be accomplished by the mixture of pigments or by allowing sunlight to pass successively through glasses of different colors, for in these cases rays corresponding to certain colors are absorbed by the medium through which the white light passes, and the phe- nomenon is the result of a process of subtraction and not addition. Light reaching the eye through red glass, for instance, looks red because all the rays except the red rays are absorbed, and light coming through green glass appeal's green for a similar reason. Now, when light is allowed to pass successively through red and green glass the only rays which pass through the red glass will be absorbed by the green. Hence no light will pass through the combi- nation of red and green glass, and darkness results. But when red and green rays are mixed by any of the three methods above described the result of this process of addition is not darkness, but a yellow color, as will be understood by reference to the color diagram on p. 334. In the case of colored pigments similar phenomena occur, for here too light reaches the eye after rays of cer- tain wave-lengths have been absorbed by the medium. This subject will be further considered in connection with color-theories.1 Color-theories. — From what has been said of color-mixtures it is evident that every color sensation may be produced by the mixture of a number of other color sensations, and that certain color sensations — viz. the purples — <':iu be produced only by the mixture of other sensations, since there is no single wave-length corresponding to them. Hence the hypothesis is a natural one that all colors are produced by the mixture in varying proportions of a certain number of fundamental colors, each of which depends for its production upon the presence in the retina of a certain substance capable of being affected (probably through some sort of a photo-chemical process) by light of a certain definite wave-length. A hypothesis of this sort lies at the basis of botli the Young-Helmholtz and the Hering theories of color sensation. The former theory postulates the existence in the retina of three substances capable of being affected by red, green, and violet rays, respectively — i. e. by the three colors lying at the three angles of the color diagram given on p. 334 1 For an interesting discussion of modem theories of color-vision, see the address of Professor Frank P. Whitman on "Color-vision," Science, Sept. 9, 1898. 336 AN AMERICAN TEXT- BOOK OE PHYSIOLOGY. — and regards all other color sensations as produced by the simultaneous affec- tion of two of these substances in varying proportions. Thus when a ray of blue light falls on the retina it stimulates the violet- and green-perceiving sub- stances, and produces a sensation intermediate between the two, while simul- taneous stimulation of the red- and green-perceiving substances produces the sensations corresponding to yellow and orange ; and when the violet- and red- perceiving substances are affected at the same time, the various shades of purple are produced. Each of these three substances is, however, supposed to be affected to a slight extent by all the rays of the visible spectrum, a suppo- sition which is rendered necessary by the fact that even the pure spectral colors do not appear to be perfectly saturated, as will be explained in connec- tion with the subject of saturation. Furthermore, the disappearance of color when objects are very feebly or very brightly illuminated or when they are seen with the lateral portions of the retina (as described on p. 333) necessitates the additional hypotheses that these three substances are all equally affected by all kinds of rays when the light is of either very small or very great intensity or when it falls on the extreme lateral portions of the retina, and that they manifest their specific irritability for red, green, and violet rays respectively only in light of moderate intensity falling not too far from the fovea centralis of the retina. The modifications of the Young- Hemholtz theory introduced by these sub- sidiary hypotheses greatly diminish the simplicity which was its chief claim to acceptance when originally proposed. Moreover, there will always remain a psychological difficulty in supposing that three sensations so different from each other as those of red, green, and violet can by their union produce a fourth sensation absolutely distinct from any of them — viz. white. The fact that in the Hering theory this difficulty is obviated has contributed greatly to its acceptance by physiologists. In this theory the retina is supposed to contain three substances in which chemical changes may be produced by ether vibrations, but each of these substances is supposed to be affected in two oppo- site ways by rays of light which correspond to complementary color sensa- tions. Thus in one substance — viz. the white-black visual substance — kata- bolic or destructive changes are supposed to be produced by all the rays of the visible spectrum, the maximum effect being caused by the yellow rays, while anabolic or constructive changes occur when no light at all falls upon the retina. The chemical changes of this substance correspond, therefore, to the sensation of luminosity as distinguished from color. In a second substance red rays are supposed to produce katabolic, and green rays anabolic changes, while a third substance is similarly affected by yellow and blue rays. These two substances are therefore spoken of as red-green and yellow-blue visual sub- stances respectively. It has been sometimes urged as an objection to this theory that the effect of a stimulus is usually katabolic and not anabolic. This is true with regard to muscular contraction, from the study of which phenomenon most of our know- ledge of the effect of stimulation has been obtained, but it should be remem- THE SENSE OF VISION. 337 bered that observations on the augmentor and inhibitory cardiac nerves have shown us that nerve-stimulation may produce very contrary effects. There seems to be, therefore, no serious theoretical difficulty in supposing that light rays of different wave-lengths may produce opposite metabolic effects upon the substances in which changes are associated with visual sensations. A more serious objection lies in the difficulty of distinguishing between the sensation of blackness, which, on Hering's hypothesis, must correspond to active anabolism of the white-black substance, and the sensation of darkness (such as we experience when the eyes have been withdrawn for some time from the influence of light), which must correspond to a condition of equilibrium of the white-black substance in which neither anabolism nor katabolism is occurring. Another objection to the Hering theory is to be found in the results of experiments in comparing grays or whites produced by mixing different colored rays under varying intensities of light. The explanation given by Hering of the production of white through the mixture of blue and yellow or of red and green is that when either of these pairs of complementary colors is mixed the anabolic and the katabolic processes balance each other, leaving the corre- sponding visual substance in a condition of equilibrium. Hence, the white- black substance being alone stimulated, the result will be a sensation of white corresponding to the intensity of the katabolic process caused by the mixed rays. Now, it is found that when blue and yellow are mixed in certain pro- portions on a revolving disk a white can be produced which will, with a certain intensity of illumination, be undistinguishable from a white produced by mix- ing red and green. If, however, the intensity of the illumination is changed, it will be found necessary to add a certain amount of white to one of the mix- tures in order to bring them to equality. On the theory that complementary colors produce antagonistic processes in the retina it is difficult to understand why this should be the case.1 A color theory which is in some respects more in harmony with recent observations in the physiology of vision has been proposed by Mrs. C. L. Franklin. In this theory it is supposed that, in its earlier periods of de- velopment, the eye is sensitive only to luminosity and not to color — i. e. it possesses only a white-black or (to use a single word) a gray-perceiving sub- stance which is affected by all visible light rays, but most powerfully by those lying near the middle of the spectrum. The sensation of gray is supposed to be dependent upon the chemical stimulation of the optic nerve-terminations by some product of decomposition of this substance. In the course of development a portion of this gray visual substance becomes differentiated into three different substances, each of which is affected by rays of light corresponding to one of the three fundamental colors of the spectrum — viz. red, green, and blue. This differentiation may be supposed to occur in the cones rather than in the rods, which thus become organs specially adapted 1 The renewal of the rod pigment in ;i dim light may afford an explanation of tins phenom- enon (see C. Ladd Franklin: Psychological Review, v. 311). Vol. II.— 22 338 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. for the perception of color (see p. 342). When a ray of light intermediate between two of the fundamental colors falls upon the retina, the visual sub- stances corresponding to these two colors will be affected to a degree pro- portionate to the proximity of these two colors to that of the incident ray. Since this effect is exactly the same as that which is produced when the retina is acted upon simultaneously by light of two fundamental colors, we are incap- able of distinguishing in sensation between an intermediate wave-length and a mixture in proper amounts of two fundamental wave-lengths. When the retina is affected by two or more rays of such wave-lengths that all three of the color visual substances are equally affected, the resulting decom- position will be the same as that produced by the stimulation of the gray visual substance out of which the color visual substances were differentiated, and the corresponding sensation will therefore be that of gray or white. It will be noticed that the important feature of this theory is that it pro- vides for the independent existence of the gray visual substance, while ?t the same time the stimulation of this substance is made a necessary result of the mixture of certain color sensations. Another color theory has recently been brought forward by Prof. G. E. Midler,1 who substitutes for Hering's antagonistic processes of assimilation and dissimilation the conception of "reversible chemical actions" — i. e. actions in which the products of a chemical change can be used for the reconstruc- tion of the original substance. Color-blindness. — The fact that many individuals are incapable of distin- guishing between certain colors — i. e. are more or less " color-blind " — is one of fundamental importance in the discussion of theories of color vision. By far the most common kind of color-blindness is that in which certain shades of red and green are not recognized as different colors. The advocates of the Young-Helmholtz theory explain such cases by supposing that either the red or the green perceiving elements of the retina are deficient, or, if present, are irritable, not by rays of a particular wave-length, but by all the rays of the visible spectrum. In accordance with this view these cases of color-blindness are divided into two classes — viz. the red-blind and the green-blind — the basis for the classification being furnished by more or less characteristic curves repre- senting the variations in the luminosity of the visible spectrum as it appears to the different eyes. There are, however, cases which cannot easily be brought under either of these two classes. Moreover, it has been proved in cases of monocular color-blindness, and is admitted even by the defenders of the Helm- holtz theory, that such persons see really only two colors — viz. blue and yellow. To such persons the red end of the spectrum appears a dark yellow, and the green portion of the spectrum has luminosity without color. A better explanation of this sort of color-blindness is given in the Hering theory by simply supposing that in such eyes the red-green visual substance is deficient or wholly wanting, but the theory of Mrs. Franklin accounts for the phenomena in a still more satisfactory way; for, by supposing that the differ- 1 7a itschriftfiir Psyehologie und Physiologie der Sinnesorgane, 1875 and 1897. THE SENSE OF VISION. 339 entiation of the primary gray visual substance has first led to the formation of a blue and a yellow visual substance, and that the latter has subsequently been differentiated into a red and a green visual substance, color-blindness is readily explained by supposing that this second differentiation has either not occurred at all or has taken place in an imperfect manner. It is, in other words, an arrest of development. Cases of absolute color-blindness occasionally occur. To such persons nature appears colorless, all objects presenting simply differences of light and shade. In whatever way color-blindness is to be explained, the defect is one of considerable practical importance, since it renders those affected by it incapable of distinguishing the red and green lights ordinarily used for signals. Such persons are, therefore, unsuitable for employment as pilots, railway engineers, etc., and it is now customary to test the vision of all candidates for employment in such situations. It has been found that no satisfactory results can be reached by requiring persons to name colors which are shown them, and the chromatic sense is now commonly tested by what is known as the " Holmgren method," which consists in requiring the individual examined to select from a pile of worsteds of various colors those shades which seem to him to resemble standard skeins of green and pink. When examined in this way about 4 per cent, of the male and one-quarter of 1 per cent, of the female sex are found to be more or less color-blind. The defect may be inherited, and the relatives of a color-blind person are therefore to be tested with special care. Since females are less liable to be affected than males, it often happens that the daughters of a color-blind person, themselves with normal vision, have sons who inherit their grandfather's infirmity. Although in all theories of color vision the different sensations are supposed to depend upon changes produced by the ether vibrations of varying rates acting upon different substances in the retina, yet it should be borne in mind that we have at present no proof of the existence of any such substances. The visual purple — or, to adopt Mrs. Franklin's more appropriate term, " the rod pigment" — was at one time thought to be such a substance, but for the reasons above given cannot be regarded as essential to vision.1 That a centre for color vision, distinct from the visual centre, exists in the cerebral cortex is rendered probable by the occurrence of cases of hemianopsia for colors, and also by the experiments of Heidenhain and Cohn on the influ- ence of the hypnotic trance upon color-blindness. Intensity. — The second of the above-mentioned qualitative modifications of light is its intensity, which is dependent upon the energy of vibrations of the molecules of the luminiferons ether. The sensation of luminosity is not, how- ever, proportionate to the intensity of the stimulus, but varies in such a way that a given increment of intensity causes a greater difference in sensation with 1 In a recently developed theory by Ebbinghaus (Zeitschrift fur Psychologic und Physiologic der Sinnesorganc, v. 145) a physiological importance in relation to vision is attached to this substance in connection with other substances of a hypothetical character. 340 AN AMERICAN TENT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. feeble than with strong illuminations. This phenomenon is illustrated by the disappearance of a shadow thrown by a candle in a darkened room on a sheet of white paper when sunlight is allowed to fall on the paper from the opposite direction. In this case the absolute difference in luminosity between the shadowed and unshadowed portions of the paper remains the same, but it becomes imperceptible in consequence of the increased total illumination. Although our power of distinguishing absolute differences in luminosity diminishes as the intensity of the illumination increases, yet with regard to relative differences no such dependence exists. On the contrary, it is found within pretty wide limits that, whatever be the intensity of the illumination, it must be increased by a certain constant fraction of its total amount in order to produce a perceptible difference in sensation. This is only a special case of a general law of sensation known as Weber's law, which has been formulated by Foster as follows : " The smallest change in the magnitude of a stimulus which we can appreciate through a change in our sensation always bears the same proportion to the whole magnitude of the stimulus." Luminosity of Different Colors. — When two sources of light having the same color are compared, it is possible to estimate their relative luminosity with considerable accuracy, a difference of about 1 per cent, of the total luminosity being appreciated by the eye. When the sources of light have different colors, much less accuracy is attainable, but there is still a great differ- ence in the intensity with which rays of light of different wave-lengths affect the retina. We do not hesitate to say, for instance, that the maximum intensity of the solar spectrum is found in the yellow portion, but it is import- ant to observe that the position of this maximum varies with the illumina- tion. In a very brilliant spectrum the maximum shifts toward the orange, and in a feeble spectrum (such as may be obtained by narrowing the slit of the spectroscope) it moves toward the green. Hence changes of intensity are associated with changes of color, and, as Haycraft l has observed, "we cannot abstract 'brightness' from our sensations of light as we can abstract 'loud- ness' from our sensations of sound." The curves in Figure 152 illus- trate this shifting of the maximum of luminosity of the spectrum with vary- ing intensities of illumination. The abscissas represent wave-lengths in millionths of a millimeter, and the ordinates the luminosity of the different colors as expressed by the reciprocal values of the width of the slit necessary to give to the color under observation a luminosity equal to that of an arbi- trarily chosen standard. The curves from A to H represent the distribution of the intensity of light in the spectrum with eight different grades of illumi- nation. This shifting of the maximum of luminosity in the spectrum explains the so-called " Purkinje's phenomenon " — viz. the changing rela- tive values of colors in varying illumination. 'Phis can be best observed at nightfall, the attention being directed to a carpet or a wall-paper the pattern of which is made up of a number of different colors. As the daylight fades away the red colors, which in full illumination are 1 " Luminosity and Photometry/' by John Berry Haycraft: Journal of Physiology, xxi. 126. THE SENSE OF VISION. 341 the most intense, becomes gradually darker, and are scarcely to be distin- guished from black at a time when the blue colors are still very readily distinguished. Function of Rods and Cones. — There is, as mentioned on p. 337, some reason to suppose that the rods and cones have different functions. That color sensation and accuracy of definition are most perfect in the central portion of the retina is shown by the fact that when we desire to obtain the best possible idea of the form and color of an object we direct 3.8- 3.6- /- :;.!■ / . I tensit, H 3.2 / \\ - ■ G 3. • / 1 / i \ * F 2.8- / / ' \ 2.6- / / v»\ — •—-.- * D 2.4. i i i i i > C » b 2.2- I j i N i i\ „ 0 a 2 i i i i \ \ 1.8- i s-^ // v *\ 1.6 // i i \ N> // \ \ 1.4- li i s\ 1.2- II i i — v J, 1. . H j i ' /•'/' ^&p% %S<'±- n.s i ■ / '•: 0.2. i f :-■- .-•' * \\ :^:"^ SSS£S5-ae, --^•S"'»-B-W~n.il 670 050 625 605 590 575 555 B C D 520 505 490 E F 470 430 a Fig. 152.— Diagram showing the distribution of the intensity of the spectrum as dependent upon the degree of illumination (Konig). our eyes in such a way that the image falls upon the fovea centralis of the retina. The luminosity of a faint object, however, seems greatest when we look not directly at it, but a little to one side of it. This can be readily observed when we look at a group of stars, as, for example, the Pleiades. When the eyes are accurately directed to the stars so as to enable us to count them, the total luminosity of the constellation appears much less than when the eyes are directed to a point a few degrees to one side of the object. Now, an examination of the retina shows only cones in the fovea centralis. In the immediately adjacent parts a small number of rods are found mingled with the cones. In the lateral portions of the retina the rods are relatively more numerous than the cones, and in the extreme peripheral portions the rods alone exist. Hence this phenomenon is readily explained on the supposition, which is supported by Kainon yCajal's1 recent observations, that the rods are a comparatively rudimentary form of visual apparatus, taking cognizance 1 Zeitschriftfiir Psychologic und Physiologic dcr Sinnesorganr, xvi. S. 161. 342 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. of the existence of light with special reference to its varying intensity, and that the cones arc organs specially modified for the localization of stimuli ami for the perception of differences of wave-lengths. The view that the rods are specially adapted for the perception of luminosity and the cones for that of color derives support from the fact that in the retina of cer- tain nocturnal animals — e. g. bats and owls — rods alone are present. This theory has been further developed by Von Kries,1 who in a recent article describes the rods as differing from the cones in the following respects: (1) They are color-blind — i. c. they produce a sensation of simple luminosity whatever be the wave-length of the light-ray falling on them ; (2) they are more easily stimulated than the cones, and are particularly responsive to light- waves of short wave-lengths; (3) they have the power of adapting themselves to light of varying intensity. On this theory it is evident that we must get the sensation of white or colorless light in two different ways : (1) In consequence of the stimulation of the rods by any sort of light- rays, and (2) in consequence of the stimula- tion of the cones by certain combinations of light-rays — i. e. complementary colors. In this double mode of white perception lies perhaps the explanation of the effect of varying intensity of illumination upon the results of color- mixtures which has been above alluded to (see p. 337) as an objection to the Hering theory. The so-ealled " Purkinje's phenomenon," described on p. 340, is readily explained in accordance with this theory, for, owing to the greater irritability of the rods, the importance of these organs, as compared with the cones, in the production of the total visual sensation is greater with feeble than with strong illumination of the field of vision. At the same time, the power of the rods to respond particularly to light-rays of short wave-length will cause a greater apparent intensity of the colors at the blue than at the red end of the spectrum. In this connection it is interesting to note that the phe- nomenon is said not to occur when the observation is limited to the fovea centralis, where cones alone are found.2 Saturation. — The degree of saturation of light of a given color depends, as above stated, upon the amount of white light mixed with it. The quality of light thus designated is best studied and appreciated by means of experiments with rotating disks. If, for instance, a disk consisting of a large white and a small red sector be rapidly rotated, the effect produced is that of a pale pink color. By gradually increasing the relative size of the red sector the pink color becomes more and more saturated, and finally when the white sector is reduced to zero the maximum of saturation is produced. It must be borne in mind, however, that no pigments represent completely saturated colors. Even the colors of the spectrum do not produce a sensation of absolute saturation, for, whatever theory of color vision be adopted, it is evident that all the color-perceiving elements of the retina are affected more or leas by all the rays of light. Thus when rays of red light fall upon the retina they will 1 Zeiischrift Jur Psychologic wnd Physiologic der Siwiesorgane, ix. 81. 2 von Kries: CentralblaitfUr Physiologie, 1896, i. THE SENSE OF VISION. 343 stimulate not only the red-perceiving elements, but to a slight extent also (to use the language of the Helmholtz theory) the green- and violet-perceiving elements of the retina. The eifect of this will be that of mixing a small amount of white with a large amount of red light — i. e. it will produce the sensation of incompletely saturated red light. This dilution of the sensation can be avoided only by previously exhausting the blue- and green-perceiving elements of the retina in a manner which will be explained in connection with the phenomena of after-images. Retinal Stimulation. — Whenever by a stimulus applied to an irritable substance the potential energy there stored up is liberated the following phe- nomena may be observed : 1. A so-called latent period of variable duration during which no effects of stimulation are manifest ; 2. A very brief period during which the effect of the stimulation reaches a maximum ; 3. A period of continued stimulation during which the effect diminishes in consequence of the using up of the substance containing the potential energy — i. e. a period of fatigue ; 4. A period after the stimulation has ceased in which the eifect slowly passes away. Fig. 153.— Diagram showing the effect of stimulation of an irritable substance. The curve drawn by a muscle in tetanic contraction, as shown in Figure 153, illustrates this phenomenon. Thus, if A D represents the duration of the stimulation, A B indicates the latent period, B C the period of contraction, C D the period of fatigue under stimulation, and 1) E the after-effect of stimulation showing itself as a slow relaxation. When light falls upon the retina corresponding phenomena are to be observed. Latent Period. — That there is a period of latent sensation in the retina (i. e. an interval between the falling of light on the retina and the beginning of the sensation) is, judging from the analogy of other parts of the nervous system, quite probable, though its existence has not been demonstrated. Rise to Maximum of Sensation. — The rapidity with which the sensation of light reaches its maximum increases with the intensity of the light and varies with its color, red light producing its maximum sensation sooner than green and blue. Consequently, when the image of a white object is moved across the retina it will appear bordered by colored fringes, since the various con- stituents of white light do not produce their maximum effects at the same time. This phenomena can be readily observed when a disk on which a black and a white spiral band alternate with each other (as shown in Figure 154, A) is rotated before the eyes. The white hand as its image moves out- ward or inward over the retinal surface appears bordered witli colors which 344 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. vary with the rate of rotation of the disk and with the amount of exhaustion of the retina. Chromatic effects due to a similar cause are also to be seen when a disk, such as is shown in Figure 154, B (known as Benham's spectrum A B Fig. 154. — Disks to illustrate the varying rate at which colors rise to their maximum of sensation. top), is rotated with moderate rapidity. The concentric bands of color appear in reverse order when the direction of rotation is reversed. The apparent movement of colored figures on a background of a different color when the eye moves rapidly over the object or the object is moved rapidly before the eye seems to depend upon this same retinal peculiarity. The phenomenon may be best observed when small pieces of bright-red paper are fastened upon a bright-blue sheet and the sheet gently shaken before the eyes. The red figures will appear to move upon the blue background. The effect may be best observed in a dimly-lighted room. In this connection should be mentioned the phenomenon of " recurrent images " or " oscillatory activity of the retina." x This may be best observed when a black disk containing a white sector is rotated at a rate of about one revolution in two seconds. If the disk is brightly illuminated, as by sunlight, and the eye fixed steadily upon the axis of rota- tion, the moving white sector seems to have a shadow upon it a short distance behind its ad- vancing border, and this shadow may be followed by a second fainter, and even by a third still fainter shadow, as shown in Figure 155. The distance of the shadows from each other and from the edge of the sector increases with the rate of rotation of the disk and corresponds to a time ro illustrate the oscillatory interval of about 0.015". It thus appears that activity of the retina (Charpentier). . when light is suddenly thrown upon the retina the sensation does not at once rise to its maximum, but reaches this point by a sort of vibratory movement. The apparent duplication of a single very brief retinal stimulation, as that caused by a flash of lightning, may perhaps be a phenomenon of the same sort. Fatigue of Retina. — When the eye rests steadily upon a uniformly illu- 1 Charpentier: Archives de Physiofogie, 1892, pp. 541, ''>•_!'.) ; and 1886, p. 677. THE SENSE OE VISION. 345 minated white surface (e. g. a sheet of white paper), we are usually unconscious of any diminution in the intensity of the sensation, but it can be shown that the longer we look at the paper the less brilliant it appears, or, in other words, that the retina really becomes fatigued. To do this it is only necessary to place a disk of black paper on the white surface and to keep the eyes steadily fixed for about half a minute upon the centre of the disk. Upon removing the disk without changing the direction of the eyes a round spot will be seen on the white paper in the place previously occupied by the disk. On this spot the whiteness of the paper will appear much more intense than on the neighboring portion of the sheet, because we are able in this experiment to bring into direct contrast the sensations produced by a given amount of light upon a fresh and a fatigued portion of the retina.1 The rapidity with which the retina becomes fatigued varies with the color of the light. Hence when intense white light falls upon the retina, as when we look at the setting sun, its disk seems to undergo changes of color as one or another of the constituents of its light becomes, through fatigue, less and less conspicuous in the combination of rays which produces the sensation of white. The After-effect of Stimulation. — The persistence of the sensation after the stimulus has ceased causes very brief illuminations (e. g. by an electric spark) to produce distinct effects. On this phenomenon depends also the above-described method of mixing colors on a revolving disk, since a second color is thrown upon the retina before the impression produced by the first color has had time enough to become sensibly diminished. The interval at which successive stim- ulations must follow each other in order to pro- duce a uniform sensation (a process analogous to the tetanic stimulation of a muscle) may be determined by rotating a disk, such as repre- sented in Figure 156, and ascertaining at what speed the various rings produce a uniform sen- sation of gray. The interval varies with the intensity of the illumination from 0.1" to 0.033", and may, therefore, be used as a measure of the intensity, as in the method of u flicker photometry." 2 The special advan- tage of this method is that it affords a means Era. m-Msk to illustrate the persistence . . ... . ... of retinal sensation (Helmholtz). 01 determining the relative intensity of lights of different colors. The duration of the after-effect depends also upon the length of the stimulation and upon the color of the light producing it. the most persistent effect being produced by the red rays. In this connection it is interesting to note that while with the rapidly vibrating blue rays a less 1 Although the retina is here spoken of as the portion of the visual apparatus subject to fatigue, it should be borne in mind that we cannot, in tin- present state of our knowledge, dis- criminate between retinal fatigue and exhaustion of tin- visual nerve-centres. 2 Rood : American Journal of Science, Sept., 1893. 346 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. intense illumination suffices to stimulate the eye, the slowly vibrating red rays produce the more permanent impression. After-images. — When the object looked at is very brightly illuminated the impression upon the retina may be so persistent that the form and color of the object are distinctly visible for a considerable time after the stimulus has ceased to act. This appearance is known as a " positive after-image," and can be best observed when we close the eyes after looking at the sun or other bright source of light. Under these circumstances we perceive a brilliant spot of light which, owing to the above-mentioned difference in the persistence of the impressions produced by the various colored rays, rapidly changes its color, passing gen- erally through bluish green, blue, violet, purple, and red, and then disappear- ing. This phenomenon is apt to be associated with or followed by another effect known as a " negative after-image." This form of after-image is much more readily observed than the positive variety, and seems to depend upon the fatigue of the retina. It is distinguished from the positive after-image by the fact that its color is always complementary to that of the object causing it. In the experiment to demonstrate the fatigue of the retina, described on p. 345, the white spot which appears after the black disk is withdrawn is the " nega- tive after-image " of the disk, white being complementary to black. If a colored disk be placed upon a sheet of white paper, looked at attentively for a few seconds, and then withdrawn, the eye will perceive in its place a spot of light of a color complementary to that of the disk. If, for example, the disk be vellow, the yellow-perceiving elements of the retina become fatigued in looking at it. Therefore when the mixed rays constituting white light are thrown upon the portion of the retina which is thus fatigued, those rays which produce the sensation of yellow will produce less effect than the other rays for which the eve has not been fatigued. Hence white light to an eye fatigued for yellow will appear blue. If the experiment be made with a yellow disk resting on a sheet of blue paper, the negative after-image will be a spot on which the blue color will appear (1) more in/ens,' than on the neighboring portions of the sheet, owing to the blue-perceiving elements of that portion of the retina not being fatigued ; (2) more saturated, owing to the yellow-perceiving elements being so far exhausted that they no longer respond to the slight stimulation which is pro- duced when light of a complementary color is thrown upon them, as has been explained in connection with the subject of saturation. Contrast. — As the eye wanders from one part of the field of vision to another it is evident that the sensation produced by a given portion of the field will be modified by the amount of fatigue produced by that portion on which the eye has last rested, or, in other words, the sensation will be the result of the stimulation l.y tl bjecl looked at combined with the negative after- image of the object previously observed. The effect of this combination is to produce the phenomenon of successive contrast, the principle of which may he thus stated : Every pari of the held of vision appears lighter near a darker part and darker near a lighter part, and its color seen near another color approaches the complementary color of the latter. A contrast phenomenon THE SENSE OF VISION. 347 similar in its effects to that above described may be produced under conditions in which negative after-images can play no part. This kind of contrast is known as simultaneous contrast, and may perhaps be explained on the theory that a stimulation of a given portion of the retina produces in the neighboring portions an effect to some extent antagonistic to that caused by direct stimulation. A good illustration of the phenomenon of contrast is given in Figure 157, in which black squares are separated by white bands which at their points of intersection appear darker than where they are bordered on either side by the black squares. A black disk on a yellow background seen through white tissue-paper appears blue, since the white paper makes the black disk look gray and the yellow background pale yellow. The gray disk in contrast to the pale yellow around it appears blue. Fig. 157.— To illustrate the phenomenon of contrast. The phenomenon of colored shadows also illustrates the principle of con- trast. These may be observed whenever an object of suitable size and shape is placed upon a sheet of white paper and illuminated from one direction by daylight and from another by gaslight. Two shadows will be produced, one of which will appear yellow, since it is illuminated only by the yellowish gas- light, while the other, though illuminated by the white light of day, will appear blue in contrast to the yellowish light around it. Space-perception. — Rays of light proceeding from every point in the field of vision are refracted to and stimulate a definite point on the sur- face of the retina, thus furnishing us with a local sign by which we can recognize the position of the point from which the light proceeds. Hence the size and shape of an optical image upon the retina enable us to judge of the size of the corresponding object in the same way that the cutane- 348 AN AMERICAN TEXT- BO OK OF PHYSIOLOGY. ous terminations of the nerves of touch enable us to judge of the size and shape of an object brought in contact with the skin. This spatial perception is materially aided by the muscular sense of the muscles moving the eyeball, for we can obtain a much more accurate idea of the size of an object if we let the eye rest in succession upon its different parts than if we gaze fixedly at a given point upon its surface. The conscious effort associated with a given amount of muscular motion gives, in the case of the eye, a measure of distance similar to that secured by the hand when we move the fingers over the surface of an object to obtain an idea of its size and shape. The perception of space by the retina is limited to space in two dimensions — i. e. in a plane perpendicular to the axis of vision. Of the third dimension in space — i. e. of distance from the eye — the retinal image gives us no know- ledge, as may be proved by the study of after-images. If an after-image of any bright object — e. g. a window — be produced upon the retina in the man- ner above described and the eye be then directed to a sheet of paper held in the hand, the object will appear outlined in miniature upon the surface of the paper. If, however, the eye be directed to the ceiling of the room, the object will appear enlarged and at a distance corresponding to that of the surface looked at.1 Hence one and the same retinal image may, under different cir- cumstances, give rise to the impression of objects at different distances. We must therefore regard the perception of distance not as a direct datum of vision, but, as will be later explained, a matter of visual judgment. When objects are of such a shape that their images may be thrown suc- cessively upon the same part of the retina, it is possible to judge of their rela- tive size with considerable accuracy, the retinal surface serving as a scale to which the images are successively applied. When this is not the case, the error of judgment is much greater. We can compare, for instance, the relative length of two vertical or of two horizontal lines with a good deal of precision, but in comparing a vertical with a horizontal line we are liable to make a con- siderable error. Thus it is difficult to realize that the vertical and the hori- zontal lines in Figure 158 are of the same length. The error consists in an over-estimation of the length of the vertical lines relatively to horizontal ones, and appears to depend, in part at any rate, upon the small size of the superior rectus muscle relatively to the other muscles of the eye. The difference amounts to 30-45 per cent, in weight and 40-53 per cent, in area of cross section. It is evident, therefore, that a given motion of the eye in the upward direction will require a more powerful contraction of the weaker muscle concerned in the movement fig. 108.-T0 illustrate the over-esti- than will be demanded of the stronger muscles mation of vertical lines. , in i moving the eye laterally to an equal amount. 1 This power of the surface of projection to determine the apparent size and distance of the after-image may be to some extent influenced by the will. — Jeffries : Journal of Boston Society of Medical Scit nces, vol. i. No. 9. THE SENSE OF VISION. 349 Hence we judge the upward motion of the eye to be greater because to accom- plish it we make a greater effort than is required for a horizontal movement of equal extent. The position of the vertical line bisecting the horizontal one (in Fig. 158) aids the illusion, as may be seen by turning the page through 90°, so as to bring the bisected line into a vertical posi- tion, or by looking at the lines in Figure 159, in which the illusion is much less marked than in Figure 158. The tendency to over-estimate the length of vertical lines is also illustrated by the error commonly made in supposing the height of the crown of an ordinarv silk hat to be greater . . , , . Fig. 159.— To illustrate the over-estima- than its breadth. tion of verticai lines. Irradiation. — Many other circumstances affect the accuracy of the spatial perception of the retina. One of the most important of these is the intensity of the illumination. All brilliantly illumi- nated objects appear larger than feebly illuminated ones of the same size, as is well shown by the ordinary incandescent electric lamp, the delicate filament of which is scarcely visible when cold, but when intensely heated by the electric current glows as a broad band of light. The phenomenon is known as " irra- diation," and seems to depend chiefly upon the above-described imperfections in the dioptric apparatus of the eye, in consequence of which points of light produce small circles of dispersion on the retina and bright objects produce Fig. 160.— To illustrate the phenomenon of irradiation. images with imperfectly defined outlines. The white square surrounded by black and the black square surrounded by white (Figure 160), being of the same size, would in an ideally perfect eye produce images of the same size on the retina, but owing to the imperfections of the eye the images are not sharply defined, and the white surfaces consequently appear to encroach upon the darker portions of the field of vision. Hence the white square looks larger than the black one, the difference in the apparent size depending upon the intensity of the illumination and upon the accuracy with which the eve can be accommo- dated for the distance at which the objects arc viewed. The effect of irradi- ation is most manifest when the dark portion of the field of vision over which the irradiation takes place has a considerable breadth. Thus the circular white 300 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY spots in Figure 161, when viewed from a distance of three or four meters, appear hexagonal, since the irradiation is most marked in the triangular dark space between three adjacent circles. A familiar example of the effect of irra- Fig. 161.— To illustrate the phenomenon of irradiation. diation is afforded by the appearance of the new moon, whose sun-illuminated crescent seems to be part of a much larger circle than the remainder of the disk, which shines only by the light reflected upon it from the surface of the earth. D E Pig. L62.— To illustrate the illusion of subdivided space. Subdivided Space. — A space subdivided into smaller portions by inter- mediate objects seems more extensive than a space of the same size not so sub- divided. Thus the distance from A to B (Fig. 1 62) seems longer than that from B to C, though both are of the same length, and for the same reason the square THE SENSE OF VISION. 351 D seems higher than it is broad, and the square E broader than it is high, the illusion being more marked in the case of D than in the case of E, because, as above explained, vertical distances are, as a rule, over-estimated. The explanation of this illusion seems to be that the eye in passing over a subdivided line or area recognizes the number and size of the subdivisions, Fig. 163.— Zollner's lines. and thus gets an impression of greater total size than when no subdivisions are present. A good example of this phenomenon is afforded by the apparently increased extent of a meadow when the grass growing on it is cut and arranged in hay- cocks.1 The relations of lines to each other gives rise to numerous illusions of spatial perception, among the most striking of which are those afforded by the so-called " Zollner's lines," an example of which is given in Figure 163. Here the horizontal lines, though strictly parallel to each ^ j * other, seem to diverge and converge alternately, their apparent direction being changed toward greater per- pendicularity to the short oblique lines crossing them. This illusion is to be explained in part by the tendency of the eye to over-estimate the size of acute and to under-estimate that of obtuse angles — a tendency which, according to Filehne,2 is due to the fact that we are constantly surrounded by square-cornered objects (houses, furniture, etc.), the right angles of which, being seen obliquely, arc projected onto our retinas as acute or obtuse angles. Knowing these angles ton. . . . i i • ,i Fig. 164.— To illustrate illusion be right angles, we are constantly applying mental of Bpace-perception. corrections to our visual data, and the habit thus acquired forces us to regard all acute and obtuse angles as nearer to right angles than they really are. The illusion in Zollner's lines is more marked when the figure is so held that the long parallel lines make an angle of about 1 It is interesting to note that a similar illusion has been observed when an interval of time subdivided by audible signals is compared with an equal interval not so subdivided (Hall and Jastrow : Mind, xi. 62). 2 Zeitschrift fur Psychologie und Physiologic drr Sinncsorgane, xvii. S. 16. 352 AN AMERICAN TEXT- BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY 45° with the horizon, since in this position the eye appreciates their real position less accurately than when they are vertical or horizontal. It is diminished, but does not disappear, when the eye, instead of being allowed Fig. 165.— To illustrate contrast in space-perception (Muller-Lyer). to wander over the figure, is fixed upon any one point of the field of vision. Hence the motions of the eye must be regarded as a factor in, but not the sole cause of, the illusion. Fig. 166.— To illustrate contrast in space-perception (Muller-Lyer). The illusion in Fig. 164, where the line d is the real and the line / the apparent continuation of the line a, is to be explained partly by the over- estimation of acute angles and partly, according to Helmholtz, by irradiation. Fig. 167.— To illustrate contrast in space-perceptiim (Muller-Lyer). The fact that the illusion is greatly diminished by turning the figure on its side seems to show that the tendency to over-estimate vertical dimensions also plays a part in its production. THE SENSE OF VISION. 353 Our estimate of the size of given lines, angles, and areas is influenced by neighboring lines, angles, and areas with which they are compared. This influence is sometimes exerted in accordance with the principle of contrast, and tends to make a given extension appear larger in presence of a smaller. and smaller in presence of a larger extension. This effect is illustrated in Fig. 168.— To illustrate so-called " confluxion " in space-perception (Muller-Lyer). Figure 165, in which the middle portion of the shorter line appears larger than the corresponding portion of the longer line, in Figure 166, in which a similar effect is observed in the case of angles, and in Figure 167, in which the space between the two squares seems smaller than that between the two oblong figure's. Fig. 169.— To illustrate so-called "confluxion" in space-perception (Muller-Lyer). In some cases, however, an influence of the opposite sort1 seems to be exerted, as is shown in Figure 168, in which the middle one of three parallel lines seems longer when the outside lines are longer, and shorter when they are shorter than it is itself, and in Figure 16S), in which a circle appears larger if surrounded by a circle larger than itself, and smaller if a smaller circle is shown concentrically within it. Fig. 170.— To illustrate the Influence of angles upon the apparent length of lines (Muller-Lyer). Lines meeting at an angle appear longer when the included angle is large than when it is small, as is shown in Figure 17<>. This influence of the included angle affords a partial explanation of the illusion shown in Figure 171, in which the horizontal line at B seems longer than at A ; but the distance 1 For this influence the name "confluxion " lias been pro posed by Miiller Lyer, from whose article in the Archivfiir Phygiologie, 1889, Sup. Bd., the above examples arc taken. Vol.. II.— 23 354 AN AMERICAN TEXT- BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. between the extremities of the oblique lines seems also to affect our estimate of Jhe horizontal line in the same way as the outside lines in Figure 168 influence our judgment of the length of the line between them. Fig. 171— Illusion of space-perception. Eintlioven1 has recently explained this phenomenon as dependent upon indistinct vision in the lateral portions of the retina which causes the blurred images of the ends of the line a to appear nearer together than those of the line h. This effect of indistinctness of outline can be illustrated by photo- graphing the lines more or less out of focus as shown in Figure 172 a. A similar explanation is given by Ein- thoven for the illusion of subdivided Space described on p. 351. Perception of Distance. — The retinal image gives us, as Ave have seen, no direct information as to the distance of the object from the eye. This knowledge is, however, quite as important as that of position in a plane perpendicular to the line of vision, and we must now consider in what way it is obtained. The first fact to be noticed is that there is a close connection be- tween the judgments of distance and of actual size. A retinal image of a given size may be produced by a small object near the eye or by a large niie at a distance from it. Hence when we know the actual size of any object (as, for example, a human figure) we judge of its distance by the size of its image on the retina. Conversely, our estimate of the actual size of an object will depend upon our judgment of its distance. The fact that children constantly misjudge both thi' size and distance of objects shows that the knowledge of this rela- tion is acquired only by experience. If circumstances mislead us with regard to the distance of an object, we necessarily make a corresponding error with regard to its size. Thus, objects seen indistinctly, as through a log, are judged to be larger, because we suppose them to be farther off than they really are. The familiar fact that the n n seems to be larger when near the horizon than when near the zenith is also an illustration of this form of illu- 1 Pfliiger's Archir, lxxi. S. 1. Pig. 172.— Illustrating Einthoven'a explanation of space illusions through indistinct vision. THE SENSE OF VISION. 355 sion. When the moon is high above our heads we have do means of esti- mating its distance from us, since there are no intervening objects with which we can compare it. Hence we judge it to be nearer than when, seen on the horizon, it is obviously farther off than all terrestrial objects. Since the size of the retinal image of the moon is the same in the two cases, we reconcile the sensation with its apparent greater distance when seen on the horizon by attributing to the moon in this position a greater actual size. If the retinal image have the form of a familiar object of regular shape — e. g. a house or a table — we interpret its outlines in the light of experience and distinguish without difficulty between the nearer and more remote parts of the object. Even the projection of the outlines of such an object on to a plane surface (?". e. a perspective drawing) suggests the real relations of the different parts of the picture so strongly that we recognize at once the relative distances of the various portions of the object represented. How powerfully a familiar outline can suggest the form and relief usually associated with it is well illus- trated by the experiment of looking into a mask painted on its interior to resemble a human face. In this case the familiar outlines of a human face are brought into unfamiliar association with a receding instead of a projecting form, but the ordinary association of these outlines is strong enough to force the eye to see the hollow mask as a projecting face.1 The fact that the pro- jecting portions of an object are usually more brightly illuminated than the receding or depressed portions is of great assistance in determining their rela- tive distance. This use of shadows as an aid to the perception of relief pre- supposes a knowledge of the direction from which the light falls on an object, and if we are deceived on the latter we draw erroneous conclusions with regard to the former point. Thus, if we look at an embossed letter or figure through a lens which makes it appear inverted the accompanying reversal of the shadows will cause the letter to appear depressed. The influence of shadows on our judgment of relief is, however, not so strong as that of the outline of a familiar object. In a case of conflicting testimony the latter usually prevails, as, for example, in the above-mentioned experiment with the mask. Aided by these peculiarities of the retinal picture, the mind interprets it as corresponding in its different parts to points at different distances from the eve, and it is interesting to notice that painters, whose work, being on a plane sur- face, is necessarily in all its parts at the same distance from (he eve, use similar devices in order to give depth to their pictures. Distant hills are painted with indistinct outlines to secure what is called "aerial perspective." Figures of men and animals are introduced in appropriate dimensions to suggest the dis- tance between the foreground and the background of the picture. Landscapes are painted preferably by morning and evening light, since at these hours the marked shadows aid materially in the suggestion of distance. 1 In the experiment the mask should he placed at a distance of about two meters and one eye closed. liven with both eyes open the illusion often persists if the distance is increased to five or six meters. 356 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. The eye, however, can ai the ear what the retina is to the eye; as the lens, cornea, etc. of the eye are simply physical media for the production of sharp images on the retina, so all parts of the organ of hearing are devoted solely to the accurate transmission of the energy of air-waves to the internal ear. The External Ear. — The pinna or auricle, commonly known simply as the " ear" (Fig. 179), is a peculiarly wrinkled sheet of tissue, consisting essen- Fig. 179.— Diagram of organ of hearing of lefl side (Quain, after Arnold): l. the pinna; 2. bottom of concha ; 2-2', meatus externus; 3, tympanum ; above 8, the chain of ossicli - : •'•'. opening into the mastoid cells; i, Eustachian tube; 5, meatus tnternus, containing the facial (uppermost) and auditory nerves; ced "ii the vestibule of the labyrinth above the fenestra ovalis; a, apex of the petrous I e; b, Internal carotid artery; c, Btyloid process; d, facial nerve, issuing from the stylo-mastoid foramen; e, mastoid process ; /, squamous pari of the bone. tiallv f Light. 36-4 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF I'HYSIOLOGY membrane and the ring of hone into which this membrane is inserted. The roof is formed by a thin plate of bone, the tegmen, which separates it from the cranial cavity, and the narrow floor, eoncave upward, is just above the jugular fossa. The cavity is lined by mucous membrane continuous with that of the Eustachian tube and the pharynx, and the membrane, like that of the Eustachian tube, i- ciliated except over the surfaces of the ossicles and the tympanic membrane. Suppurative inflammation of the middle ear may not only involve the mastoid cells, but may also cause absorption of the thin plate of bone forming the roof of the tympanic cavity and the mastoid antrum. In this and in other ways inflammation may extend from the tym- panic to the cranial cavity, making otitis media, or inflammation of the middle ear, the commonest source of pyogenic affections of the brain.1 Tympanic Membrane, or Drum-skin. — The membrana tympani (Figs. 181, 182) is a somewhat oval disk whose longer axis is directed from behind and above downward and forward, and whose length is about nine millimeters. The membrane is inserted obliquely to the axis of the auditory canal, so that the floor of the canal 5 is longer than its roof. The membrana tympani, though 8 so thin as to be semi-trans- parent, is composed of three layers of tissue. Externally it is covered by a thin plate 12 of skin ; internally, by mu- ll cons membrane ; a nd between these lies the proper sub- \P*~y£ Fig. 182.*— Tympanum of righl Bide with ossicles in place, viewed in. in u itlun (after Morris) : I, body of incus ; 2, suspensory ligament of malleus; :;, ligament of Incus; i, head of malleus; .".. epitym- panic cavity; 6, chorda tympani nerve; 7, tendon ol tensor tympani muscle; 8, fool pie< f stirrup; 9, ■ ■ ,• 10, manubrium; U, tensor tympani muscle; 12, membrana tympani; i"., Eustachian tuiir. Fig. 188.— The chain of auditory OBsicles, anterior view (after TeB- tut) : l, head of malleus; 'J, long process of incus; 3, stapes. stance (membrana propia) of the membrane, made up chiefly of fibrous tissue. The greater number of the fibres of the membrana propria radiate from near the centre to the periphery of the membrane; but there are also circular fibres of elastic tissue which are most numerous in a ring near the attached margin of the membrane. The Burface of the tympanic membrane is not flat, but is funnel-shaped, with the apex of the funnel pointing inward. Moreover lines JMacewen: Pyogenic Diseases of the Brain ami Spinal ('mil, l.S'.t:;. Figs. 180, L81,and 1 82 are taken by permission from Morris's Text-Book of Anatomy, Phila., 1893. TEE SENSE OF HEARING. 365 drawn from the centre to the margin of the membrane would not be straight, but would be curved slightly, with the convexity outward, this shape being due to the tension of the elastic circular fibres of the membrane. The mem- brane, throughout the greater part of its circumference, is inserted in a groove in a bony ring set in the wall of the auditory canal, but a small arc at its superior portion is attached directly to the wall of the canal. The segment of membrane corresponding to this arc, known as the membrana flaccida, lacks the tenseness of the rest of the drum-skin. Viewed through the aural speculum, the normal tympanic membrane has a pearly lustre (Fig. 181). The handle of the malleus, or manubrium, inserted within its fibrous layer, can be seen as an opaque ridge running from near the upper anterior margin downward and backward and ending in the umbo, or central depression, where the membrane is drawn considerably inward by the tip of the manubrium. It is from this point that the radial fibres of the mem- brana propria diverge. At the top of the manubrium is a shining spot which is the reflection from the short process of the malleus where it presses against the membrane. From this point two delicate folds of the membrane run to the periphery — one forward and the other backward. They form the lower border of the membrana flaccida, or ShrapneWs membrane, in which there is less fibrous tissue than in the remaining part of the membrane, and the cutaneous and mucous layers are also less tense than elsewhere. A bright reflection of triangular shape, known as the " pyramid of light," is seen in the lower quadrant of the tympanic membrane. The apex of this bright triangle is at the tip of the manu- brium, and its base is on or near the periphery of the membrane. Auditory Ossicles. — The tympanic membrane is put into relation with the internal ear by a chain of bone, the auditory ossicles, known as the malleus, the incus, and the stapes, so called from their fancied resemblance to a hammer, an anvil, and a stirrup (Figs. 1K0, 182, 183). The malleus (Fig. 184) is 18 to 19 milli- meters long; it presents a rounded head, ° ' . . Fig. 184.— Maueus of the right side : a, anterior grooved on one side for articulation with face; B.internal face (after Testat): l, capita- ,i i i i i I ii him or head of malleus; 2, cervix or neck; 8. the incus, a short neck, and a long handle procesem8 ,,n.vis: , pr !88tM gracilis; 5, manu' or manubrium, which is inserted in the brium; 6, grooved articular surface for incus ; . „ . . . .. 7, tendon of m. tensor tympani. tissue of the tympanic membrane from a point on its upper periphery to a little below its centre The processus brevis of the malleus is a low conical projection which springs from the top of the manubrium and presses directly against that segment of the tympanic membrane known as the membrana flaccida, through which it can be seen shining on inspection with the ear-speculum. The processus gracilis, or pro- ;;.;.; AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF fUYSfOLOGY cessus Folia nits, long and slender, arises from an eminence just below the neck of the malleus, and, passing forward and outward, is inserted in the Glaserian fissure in the wall of the tympanum. The malleus is held in posi- ti hi partly by ligaments; the suspensory or superior ligament passes downward and outward from the roof of the tympanum to be inserted into the head of the malleus. The main portion of the anterior ligament is attached to the neck of the malleus just above the processus gracilis ; it embraces the latter, and, passing forward, finds its origin in the anterior wall of the tympanum and in the Glaserian fissure. Another division of this ligament, the external ligament, arises and is attached more externally than that just described. The ligaments of the malleus serve to keep its head in position. The exter- nal ligament, being attached above the axis of rotation of the hammer, pre- vents the head of this bone from moving too far inward, and the manu- brium from being pushed too far outward. The superior ligament, owing to its oblique course, restrains the head of the hammer from moving too far outward. The incus, umbos, or anvil-bone (Fig. 186) is shaped somewhat like a bicus- pid tooth. Its thicker portion is hollowed on the surface and is covered with cartilage for articulation with the head of the malleus. It has two processes, a long and a short, which project at right angles to Fig. 185.— ligaments of the ossicles and their axis of rotation (from Foster, after Bensen). The figure represents a nearly horizontal section of tin- tym- panum, carried through tin- heads of the malleus and Incus: M, malleus; I, incus ;t, articular tooth of incus: lii.n and lg.e, external ligament of mal- leus : Ig.inc, ligament of the incus ; the line a-x rep- resents the axis of rotation of the two ossicles. Fig. 1S6.— The incus of the right side : \, anterior face; B, Internal face (after Testut): 1. body of incus; 2, processus brevis; :;, processus longus ; 1, articular rurface for the mal- leus; 5, a convex tubercle, processus lenticularis, fur articu- lation with stapes; i',, rough surface for attachment of the ligament of the incus. FlG. 187.— The stapes (after Testut): 1, base; 2, anterior cms; S, posterior cms; I, articulating surface of head of the bone ; 5, cervix or neck. each other; the former has a length of A\ millimeters, and the latter a length of 3 to •'>.', millimeters. When in position the long process descends nearly parallel with the manubrium, but it has less than three-fourths the length of the latter. The i'veo end of the long process is turned sharply inward at right angles, and terminates in a round projection, the 08 orbiculare, which is provided with cartilage for articulation with the head of the stapes. The short process is THE SENSE OF HEARING. 367 conical in shape and is thicker than the long process. It has a horizontal posi- tion, and is attached by a thick ligament to the posterior wall of the tympanum. The stapes (Fig'. 187) articulates with the end of the long process of the incus ; its plane is horizontal and about at right angles to that process. It measures 3 to 4 millimeters in length and about 2J millimeters in breadth. The base of the stapes is somewhat oval in shape, the superior margin being convex and the inferior being slightly concave. It is set in the fenestra ovalis, an aperture measuring about 3 millimeters by 1| millimeters, and is held in place by a narrow membrane made up of radial fibres of connective tissue. When in position, the inner face of the base of the stirrup is covered with lymphatic endothelium and is washed by the perilymph of the internal ear; the outer face, like the other tympanic bones and the wall of the cavity, is covered by thin mucous membrane. Movement of the Ossicles. — The malleus-incus articulation is so arranged that with outward movements of the manubrium the head of the malleus glides freely in the joint ; but the lower margins of the articulating surfaces project in such a way that the prominences lock together when the manubrium moves inward. Thus, in inward movements of the tympanic membrane and its attached manubrium, the malleus and the incus move together like one rigid piece of bone, the motions of the manubrium and the long process of the incus being parallel. Of the malleus-incus articulation Helraholtz1 says: u In its action it may be compared with the joints of the well-known Breguet watch-keys, which have rows of interlocking teeth, offering scarcely any resist- ance to revolution in one direction, but allowing no revolution whatever in the other." In the outward movements the locking teeth or projections are prob- ably still kept in apposition, under ordinary circumstances, through the clastic reaction of the ligament and the stapedial attachment of the incus. Should, however, the tympanic membrane be forced unduly outward, as by increase of pressure within the tympanum or by rarefaction of air in the auditory meatus, the incus only follows the malleus for a certain distance, the latter completing its motion by gliding in the joint. There is thus no danger of the stapes being torn out of the oval window. The hammer and the anvil, suspended by their ligaments, move freely about an axis one end of which is found at the origin of the anterior part of the anterior ligament of the malleus, and the other end in the origin of the ligament which is continuous with theshort process of the incus ( Fig. 185). In inward motions of the tympanic membrane the ossicles move like a single bone around the axis of suspension j and as the distance measured from the axis of rotation to the tip of the manubrium, where the power is applied, is about one and one-half times the distance to the end of the long process of the incus, where the effect is produced, the motions transmitted to the st;ipes can have but two-thirds the amplitude of the movements of the tip of the manubrium, but have one and one-half times their force. It will be noticed thai a large pro- portion of the mass of both anvil and hammer is found above their axis of rota- tion ; this upper portion acts as a counterpoise to the parts below which arc directly 1 Sensations of Tone, trans, by Kllis, ISS'i, p. l.'W. 368 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. concerned in the Lever action. The bony lever being thus balanced, it is less difficult to understand its known sensitiveness to impulses that are inconceivably weak. The tense tympanic membrane, by reason of its funnel shape, resists Strong inward compression; hence the stapes is prevented from being pressed too far inward. The maximum amplitude of motion of the stapes in the fenestra is very small, being only about ,'s millimeter to y^ millimeter, while that of the centre of the tympanic membrane is about -^ millimeter to ^ millimeter. The functional movements of the auditory ossicles are not molecular but are molar vibrations, the chain of bones moving in a body. The sole purpose of this apparatus of the middle ear is to transmit exactly the variations of pressure in the air of the external auditory meatus to the perilymph which bathes the foot of the stapes — in other words, to convert air-waves into a similar series of water-waves. In the words of Helmholtz,1 "The mechanical problem which the apparatus within the drum of the ear had to solve was to transform a motion of great amplitude and little force, such as impinges on the drum-skin, into a motion of small amplitude and great force, such as had to be communicated to the fluid in the labyrinth." The adaptation of the apparatus of the middle ear to this end is worthy of careful consideration. In the first place, it will be noticed that the area of the fenestra ovalis which receives the impulses of the stapes is but a small fraction of the surface of the tympanic membrane on which the air-waves impinge, the latter area being some fifteen to twenty times greater than the former, so that the energy of air-motion is, in a fashion, concentrated. In the second place, as previously observed, the lever mechanism of the auditory ossicles is such that the movements of the end of the long process of the incus have two-thirds the amplitude of those of the tip of the manubrium, but about one and one-half times their force. It should also be noticed that the membrane fastening the foot of the stapes in the fenestra is somewhat less tense on the upper side, so that the top of the oval foot-piece has a freer motion than the bottom, and the head of the stirrup rises slightly with inward motions. In the third place, it has been demonstrated by Helmholtz2 that the shape of the tympanic membrane peculiarly adapts it for transforming weak movements of wide amplitude into strong ones of small compass. For this membrane is not a simple funnel depressed inwardly, but the radii are slightly curved with the convexity outward, a shape chiefly due to the tension of the elastic circular fibres of the membrane on its inner face, these being most numerous toward the circumference. Air-wave- beating upon this convexity flatten the curve somewhat, and their whole energy must be concentrated, with increased intensity but loss of motion, at the central point of the membrane. This effect may be illustrated by holding a slightly-curved brass wire, several inches in length, with its plane perpendicular to the surface of a table and supported on it- ends. When one end of the wire is held immovable, up-and- down motions of the arch are transferred to the free end with diminished 1 Op. cit., p. 134. 2 Op. cit. TEE SENSE OF HEARING. 369 amplitude. The wire represents a single radial fibre of the tympanic mem- brane, and the funnel shape of this membrane is adapted to concentrating this motion of the radial fibres upon the manubrium. The same effect is illus- trated by the fact that when a string or a rope is stretched between two points, no matter how tightly, it always sags at its middle; the weight of the cord, however slight, is sufficient to give it a curved course, and produces a corre- sponding traction on the points of support. Eustachian Tube. — That the tympanic membrane may maintain its freedom of motion, it is obviously necessary that the average atmospheric pressure on each side of it should remain the same. This equality of pressure is maintained through the medium of the Eustachian tube, a somewhat trumpet- shaped canal which, beginning in the lower anterior walls of the tympanum, runs downward, forward, and inward, and terminates in a slit in the side of the upper part of the pharynx. The Eustachian tube is lined, like the walls of the tympanum, with ciliated epithelium, the cilia working in such a way as to carry into the pharynx such secretions as may arise from the mucous membrane of the middle ear. The pharyngeal opening of the Eustachian tube is probably normally closed, but it may easily be made to open by increase or decrease of air-pressure within the pharynx, as may be produced by closing the nose and mouth and either forcing air into the pharynx by strong expiration or rarefying it by suction. In the former case the air-pressure within the tympanum is increased, and in the latter it is diminished. When air is thus made to enter or to leave the tympanum, a sensation of a sudden snap and a dull crackling noise in the ear is experienced. The lower end of the tube is normally opened during the act of swallowing, and it is at this moment that the intra- and extra-tympanic air-pressures are equalized. Muscles of the Middle Ear. — Two muscles are devoted to adjusting the tension of the auditory mechanism of the middle ear. The tensor tympani is lodged within a groove which is just above and about parallel with the Eusta- chian tube. It terminates externally in a long tendon which bends nearly at right angles round the outer edy;e of the groove and is inserted into the handle of the malleus near the neck. Contraction of the tensor tympani thus pulls inward the tympanic membrane, increases its tension, and some- what dampens its vibrations. At the same time a strain is put upon the chain of ossicles, the toothed processes of the malleus and incus arc broughl more closely together, and the foot of the stapes is pressed into the oval win- dow, increasing the pressure upon the fluids of the internal ear. It is said that the relaxed tympanic membrane, particularly alter section of the tensor tympani muscle, is thrown into sympathetic vibration with comparative ease, and is in this condition best adapted to respond to weak aerial impulses and to (he periodic waves of musical notes. When the membrane is tense its vibrations are damped, and it is particularly lilted to transmit noises and con- sonantal sounds, and thus the muscle involved would seem important to the clear transmission of ordinary speech, though its effect would lie to decrease the acuteness of hearing. According to Hensen,1 the tensor tympani muscle 1 Hermann's Handbuch der Physiologic, 1880. Vol. TT.— 24 370 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. is excited to reflex contraction by the initial waves of a sound, resulting in a closer union of the toothed processes of the malleus and incus, so that there is Less loss of motion in the subsequent vibrations. But Ostermann l believes the muscle to be chiefly a protective mechanism which by its contraction prevents oscillation of so wide an amplitude as to be hurtful, and that its reflex action is called forth chiefly by very loud noises (PL 1, Fig. 1). The stapedius is a small muscle imbedded in the inner wall of the tympanum, near the fenestra ovalis. Its tendon, passing forward, is inserted into the neck' of the stapes. Contraction of the muscle would cause a slight rotation of the stapes round a vertical axis, so that the hinder part of the foot of the ossicle would be pressed more deeply into the fenestra, while the remaining portion would be drawn out of it. Its action probably reduces the pressure in the cavity of the perilymph, and thus is antagonistic to that of the tensor tympani (PL 1, Fig. 2, A, b). Vibrations of the Tympanic Membrane. — It is a general physical law that every elastic body can be made to vibrate more easily at one definite rate than at any other. The musical tone represented by this rate of vibration is known as the prime or fundamental tone of the body. Membranes have funda- mental tones (see p. 383), whose pitch is determined by their area, thickness, and tension, but they differ from rods and strings in being less strictly confined to a single fundamental tone in their vibration. The tympanic membrane is quite peculiar in that it can hardly be said to have a definite fundamental tone. It would obviously be a great imperfection in an organ of hearing were cer- tain sounds intensified by it out of proportion do others, as would be the case if the tympanic membrane had a marked fundamental tone of its own. This is prevented in the case of the membrana tympani probably both by reason of the peculiar form of its surface and its structure, and also because its oscilla- tions are damped by the pressure of the malleus held in position by the other mechanisms of the tympanum. When the tympanic membrane is perforated or is wholly removed, without destructive inflammatory changes in the middle ear, sounds are still heard, though usually with diminished loudness. A musician who had suffered this accident was no longer able to play his violin, probably because sounds of different pitch ceased to be perceived in their true relations of loudness. We may thus conclude that the function of the tym- panic membrane is not only to guard against injury to the delicate mem- branes of the fenestra and the internal ear, but also to transmit to the ossicles sonorous vibrations with their true proportion of intensity. The membranes covering the round and oval windows of the internal ear have no means of damping sympathetic vibrations (see p. 385), and, should complex air-waves strike directly upon them, they would, probably, by sympathetic resonance, respond more powerfully to tones of certain pitch than to any others. The sensation of sound may be excited by conduction through the bones of the skull as well as in the ordinary way. Thus, a tuning-fork set vibrating and held between the teeth or on the forehead is heard perfectly, and more 1 Archiv fur Anatomic a, a! Physiologic, 1898, S. 75. THE SENSE OF HEARING. 371 loudly when the ears are closed than when open. The vibrations thus con- ducted probably partly affect the internal ear directly, and partly indirectly bv setting in oscillation the tympanic membrane. It is said that when the sound of a tuning-fork held close to the ear dies away, it may again be heard if the handle of the fork be pressed against the teeth. When the tone now fails, it once more becomes audible if one of the ear-passages is lightly closed, and the sound seems to be on the side which is closed. The sensation failing, it may again be aroused if the appropriately formed handle of the fork be inserted in the auditory meatus.1 Normal individuals differ greatly in their keenness of hearing, and tests show frequently disparity in the sensibility of the two ears. The hearing ability of children is said to improve up to the age of twelve years. There is no functional relation between keen hearing and sensibility to pitch.2 The Internal Ear, or Labyrinth. — The internal ear is the site of the true organ of hearing. The membranous labyrinth (PI. 1, Fig. 4 ; Fig. 191) is a com- plicated system of membranous tubes and sacs, in which terminate at particular points the filaments of the auditory nerve ; it is contained within a chamber, the bony labyrinth, hollowed out in the petrous bone. The cavity of the bony labyrinth (Figs. 188, 189) consists of a median part, the vestibule, which is pro- longed posteriorly in the system of semicircular canals and anteriorly in the cochlea. The vestibule is a space which measures about oue-fifth of an inch in diameter, and it is perforated in its outer wall by an oval opening known as the fenestra ovalis. The semicircular canals are three tubes of circular Fig. 188.— Right bony labyrinth, viewed from outer side: the figure represents the appearance produced by removing the petrous bone down to the denser layer Immediately surrounding the labyrinth (from Quain, after Sdmmering): 1, 2,3, the superior, posterior, and horizontal semicir- cular canals; 4, 5, 6, the ampullae of the same; 7, the vestibule; 8, the fenestra ovalis; 9, fenestra rotunda ; 10, first turn of the cochlea ; 11, second turn ; 12, apex. Fig. 189.— Interior view of left bony labyrinth after removal of the superior and external trails (from Quain, after Sdmmering): I, '-', ::, the superior, pos- terior, and horizontal semicircular canals; i, fovea hemi-elliptica ; 5, fovea hemispherical 6, common opening of the superior and posterior semicircular canals ; 7, opening of the aqueduct of the vestibule ; 8, opening of the aqueduct <>f the cochlea; '.'. the scala vestihuli ; lo.seala tympani ; the lamina spiralis separating 9 and 10. section, known respectively as the anterior or superior, the posterior, and the 1 Rinne, quoted by Ilensen : Hermann'* Hindi, , ush ,/,,- Physiologie, 1880, Bd. iii. Tli. 'J, S. 26. 2 Seashore: "Studies in Psychology," Bulletin University of Iowa, 1899. :;7i' AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. external or horizontal semicircular canal. Their planes are at right angles to one another, so that they occupy the three possible dimensions of space. The externa] canal lies in a nearly horizontal plane, while the other two approach the vertical. Each canal is dilated at one extremity into a globular cavity which is more than twice the diameter of the canal itself, and which is known as the am- putta. The anterior and posterior canals unite near the ends not provided with am- pulla1, and they enter the vestibule as a com- mon tube. Anteriorly the cavitv of the — 4 vestibule is continued as a tube of complex internal structure which is coiled upon itself l'i'.. 1'."'.- I tiiiLT.-iin lift he osseous cochlea _ _ l laid open (after Quain) : I, scala vestibuli ; two and one-half times, and which, from its LMamina spiralis. 3 scala tympani ; 4, cen- regemblance to the ghell of a snai] jg kncnyn tral pillar or modiolus. ' as the cochlea (PI. 1, Fig. 3). The osseous cochlea may be conceived as formed by a bony tube turned about a bony central pillar, the modiolus, which diminishes in diameter from the base to the apex of the cochlea. From the modiolus a bony shelf stretches into the cavity of the tube, incompletely dividing it into two tubular chambers, winding round the modiolus like a circular staircase, the upper of which chambers we shall —9 Fig. 191— Diagram of right membranous labyrinth seen from the external side (after Testut) : 1, utri- cle ; 2, 3, 4, superior, posterior, and horizontal semicircular canals; 5, saccule: 6, ductus endolymphat- lcus, with 7, 7', its twigs of origin : 8, Baccus endolymphaticus : 9, canalis cochlearis, with 9', its vestibular cul-de-sac, and 9", its blind extremity ; 10, canalis reunions. soon learn to know as the scala vestibuU, and the lower chamber as the scala tympani ( Fig. 1 J»0; PI. 1, Fig. 3). The bony shelf mentioned above as partly bisecting the cochlear tube has, of course, like the latter, a spiral course, and is known as the lamina spiralis; its importance as a supporter of the auditory- nerve filaments will soon be seen. Contained within the cavitv of the bony labyrinth, and parallel with its walls, is the membranous labyrinth, in which are found the essential structures of the organ of hearing (PI. 1, Fig. 4 ; Fig. 11*1 ). The membranous labyrinth is filled with a somewhat watery, mucin-holding fluid, the endolymph, while a similar fluid, the perilymph, is found outside it and within the osseous labyrinth. The Explanation of Plate 1. Fig. 1.— Schematic representation of displacement of the auditory ossicles due to contraction of the tensor tympani muscle (Testut): a, external auditory meatus; 6, tympanic cavity; c, vestibule of the bony labyrinth ; , ductus endolymphaticus with its two branches of origin ; 6, saceus endolymph- aticus ; 7, canalis reuniens, or canal of Hensen ; 8, scala tympani . <», scala vestibuli; 10, their communi- cation at the helicotrema ; 11, aqua-ductus vestibuli ; 12, aqua ductus cochlearis : 13, periosteum ; 14, dura mater; 15, stapes in the fenestra ovalis ; Ifi, fenestra rotunda w itli its membrane. THE SENSE OF HEARING. Plate 1. THE SENSE OF HEARING. 373 perilymph space, which is lined by lymphatic epithelium, is in communication, along the sheath of the auditory nerve, with the subdural and subarachnoid lymph-areas of the brain. Numerous sheets and bars of connective tissue cross from the wall of the bony to that of the membranous labyrinth and help support the latter. That part of the membranous labyrinth lying within the vestibule is composed of two separate sacs — a larger posterior, known as the utricle or utriculus, and a smaller, more anterior, known as the saccule or sacculus. The plane of division between the two sacs ends opposite the fenestra oval is (PI. 1, Fig. 4). Though the sacs are quite separate, their cavities are indirectly continu- ous, through the union of two small tubes arising from either sac, which tubes unite to form the ductus endolymphaticus, a tube running inward through a canal in the petrosal bone and ending blindly in a dilated flattened extremity, the saccus endolymphaticus, this being supported between the layers of the dura mater within the cavity of the skull (PI. 1, Fig. 4). Bundles of audi- tory-nerve fibres penetrate the wall of each sac. The utricle gives rise to the membranous semicircular canals, which communicate with it at five points, it being remembered that the anterior and posterior canals fuse into a single tube at the ends not provided with ampulla?, and that they have a common entrance into the utricle. The saccule is continuous by a narrow tube, the canalis reuniens, with that division of the membranous labyrinth contained within the cochlea and known as the canalis cocJdearis. The auditory nerve really consists of two distinct divisions having separate origins and different distributions. One of these branches passes finally to the cochlea, and the other to the vestibule and the semicircular canals. The nerve approaches the labyrinth by way of a canal known as the meatus auditorius internus, and on reaching the angle between the vestibule and the base of the cochlea the cochlear division passes to the cochlea. The remainder of the nerve consists of two divisions, the superior of which is distributed to the utricle and to the ampullae of the anterior and horizontal semicircular canals; the inferior branch supplies the saccule and the posterior semicircular canal. The inner wall of both utricle and saccule is developed at a particular spot into a low mound, the macula acustica, made up of an accumulation of the connective-tissue ele- ments of the membranous wall and covered by a peculiarly modified epithe- lium, the auditory epithelium | Fig. 192). All the auditory-nerve filaments that enter the saccule and utricle respectively pass to these mounds and there enter into relation with the auditory epithelium. As the auditory-nerve endings are confined to a particular area in the utricle and the saccule, so the nerve-fibres supplying the semicircular canals are limited to a certain part of the ampulla of each canal. The tissue of the w;ill of the ampulla is developed into a ridge projecting into the cavity in a direction across its long axis. Tins ridge, present in each ampulla, is called the crista acustica; it is capped by a thick layer of columnar epithelial cells, the auditory epithelium, which thins away ;it the border of the crista into the sheet of flattened cells by which the rest of the ampulla is lined. The auditory cells (Fig. 192) are said to be of two kinds — one, cylindrical in 374 AX .\Mi:ni(AX TEXT-HOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. Pig. 192.— Diagram showing the epithelial cellsof a macula or a crista (after Foster): 1, cylinder or hair-cell ; 2, the same, enveloped in a nest of nerve- fibrils; 3, 4, 5, forms of rod- or spindle-culls. shape and reaching only pan way to the basement membrane, the hair-cells; the other, aarrow and elongated, the supporting or mxientaeular cells. The former are peculiar in the fact that from their free ends there project long, -till*, hair-like processes. The filaments of the ampullary-nerve branches puss through the crista and encircle the bodies of the hair-cells. The cells /covering the maculae acustica have / essentially the same structure as those just described, though in the maculae the auditory hair- are shorter than in the eristic. Seated on the free surface of the macular epithelium is a fibrous mass which is said to be a normal structure, and not, like a somewhat similar mass found covering the crista' in post-mortem section, a coagulum due to the method of preparation. Imbedded in the membrane over the maculae of both saccule and utricle are small crystals, otoliths or oto- conia, composed chiefly of carbonate of lime. Otoconia are also found less constantly in the ampullae and even in the perilymph space of the cochlea. In fishes there are large masses of calcareous matter, otoliths, attached to the wall of the auditory sac. General Anatomy of the Cochlea. — By far the most complex structure of the ear is found in the cochlea (PI. 1. Figs. 1, 3, 4 ; Figs. 188-191). The bony cochlea continues from the anterior wall of the vestibule, and in the upright posi- tion of the head the axis of the modiolus is nearly horizontal, pointing, from base to apex, outward and slightly down and forward, the base of the cochlea being formed by the inner surface of the petrous bone. The membranous cochlea, canalis or ductus cochlearis, is a tube of nearly triangular cross-section which winds round the modiolus from base to apex | Fig. 193). The base or outer side of this triangle is attached closely to the bony wall of the cochlea; the upper side, supposing the modiolus to be vertical with its apex above, is made of a thin sheet of cells known as the membrane of Reissner ; the lower side is made up partlv of the bony margin of the lamina spiralis and partly of a membrane, radially striated, stretched across from the edge of the spiral lamina to the side wall of the cochlea; this i- called the basilar membrane, iiir the cupola. The Transmission of Vibrations through the Labyrinth. — Vibrations of the tympanic membrane are transmitted as pulses of very small amplitude to the membrane covering the fenestra ovalis. The relatively considerable body of perilymph bathing the inner face of this membrane must be thus set in motion, and there starts a fluid-wave which is free to make its way throughout the perilymph-spaces of the vestibule and the semicircular canals. It may pass from the vestibule along the seala vestibuli to its top, through the helicotrema, and back by way of the seala tympani, at whose bottom it finally surges against the membrane covering the fenestra rotunda; or the wave may be transmitted directly across the membranous cochlea. The fluids of the laby- rinth being physically incompressible, the function of the fenestra rotunda as a sort of safety-valve seems evident. Politzer inserted a glass tube in the round window, and found that fluid in the tube rose when strong air-pressure was brought to bear on the outer side of the tympanic membrane. The cavity of the membranous labyrinth (PI. 1, Fig. I) is nowhere in communication with the perilymph -space about it, and we must therefore assume that the irritation of the auditory cells seated in its wall must depend on vibrations transmitted from the perilymph directly through the membranous sacs and tubes. Like the peri lymph -space, the cavity of the membranous labyrinth is in communication throughout, though in certain situations the connection of adjacent parts is very indirect. Thus, though the semicircular canals open freely at both ends into the utricle, the utricle and saccule are only brought into union by the two narrow tubes that unite to form the ductus endolym- phaticus. It will be noted that by means of this duct the membranous laby- rinth is really continued into the cranial cavity. The saccnle in turn is continuous with the seala media of the cochlea by way of the canalis reuniens. The Membranous Cochlea and the Organ of Corti ( Figs. 193-li)o).— The cochlear division of the auditory nerve, together with the nutrient blood- vessels, penetrates the modiolus at its base and runs up through the spongy interior of the bony pillar. As the nerve ascends through the modiolus its fibres are gradually all diverted to run in a radial direction between the bony plates of the lamina spiralis, to terminate in the organ of <'<>r/i of the canalis cochlearis. A collection of nerve-cells is interposed in the course of the audi- tory fibres at the base of the lamina spiralis. A complete view of the nerves of the cochlea would show a central pillar of nerve-fibres diminishing in thickness from below upward, and winding round this pillar a spiral sheet of radially-disposed nerve-fibres containing, near their point of departure from the central pillar, a spiral line of ganglion- cells; this collection of cells is therefore known as the gang/ion spirale. The THE SENSE OF HEARING. 377 thin, free edge of the bony lamina spiralis is, in the recent state, thickened bv a development of connective tissue forming a promontory known as the limbics. The free edge of the limbics is in turn shaped in such a way as to make a short, sharp projection in the plane of the upper surface of the lamina and a longer projection in the plane of its lower surface, leaving the free margin between them hollowed out. The upper projection, which is known as the vestibular lip, labium vestibvlarei serves for the attachment of the tectorial membrane, membrana tedoria, presently to be described. The lower projection is called the tympanic lip (labium tympanicum) ; to it is attached the inner margin of the basilar membrane, on whose inner half is seated the very complex struct- ure known as the organ of Corti. The basilar membrane is a thin sheet of fibrillated connective tissue stretched tightly between the tympanic lip of the limbns on the inside and the spiral ligament (see p. 379) on the outside. The more median part of the membrane, which supports the organ of Corti, is thin and rigid and is fibrillated in a radial direction. The outer part, which is first thicker and then thinner again near its point of attachment, is distinctly composed of radial fibres cemented together ; the isolated fibres are characterized by being stiff and brittle. The organ of Corti (Figs. 193, 194) has as its supporting basis a series of peculiarly modified epithelial cells, known as the rods of Cord (Fig. 195, B, r.'), which are disposed along the edge of the spiral lamina in two rows, an inner and an outer. The inner rods have their feet on the basilar membrane near its median attachment; they lean outward and upward, and at their upper extrem- ity join or articulate with the heads of the outer rods, whose feet are fastened to the basilar membrane more externally. The two rows of rods are thus joined together like the rafters of a house, and enclose beneath them a canal known as the tunnel of the organ of Corti. The inner rods are more numerous than the outer, so that the latter are fastened rather between than to the ends of the former. Leaning against the inner or median side of the inner row of rods is a single row of hair-cells (Fig. 19 1), much like those described as seated on the maculae and crista? of the labyriuth, to which hair-cells filaments of the auditory nerve are distributed. Closely applied to the .-ingle row of hair- cells, on the inner side, are several rows of columnar cells gradually decreas- ing in size toward the median line, and beneath the whole is a group of nuclei. External to the outer row of rods, and separated from it by a space, are four parallel rows of hair-cells known as the cells of Corti; their bodies do not reach downward as far as the basilar membrane, and jusi below each row is a bundle of nerve-fibres which have traversed the tunnel of Corti and then have changed their direction from a radial t<» a longitudinal or spiral one. These fibres, and others having a more direci course, one by one end in clusters encircling the individual hair-cells. Four rows of peculiarly-modified columnar cells, the cells of Deiters, are inserted closely between the cells of Corti, the OUtermosI row being external to the fourth row of Corti. These cells rest below on the basilar membrane. Still external to these groups of cells is a series of rows of tall columnar cells 378 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY of simple character supported upon the basilar membrane, and rapidly decreas- ing in heighl externally into a layer of cuboidal epithelium covering the outer part of the basilar membrane. The rods of < lorti arc peculiarly shaped at the top, the upper extremity of each being bent at an angle so as to project exter- nally and parallel with the basilar membrane; these projections are the pha- la/ngar processes of the rods, the phalanges of the inner row overlapping those of the outer row. These phalangar processes of the rods form the points of attachment — in fact, the beginning — of the reticulate membrane (inembrana reticulata), a peculiar cuticular, network-like structure formed of rings and cross-bars, having the appearance of certain vegetable tissues seen under the microscope. The reticulate membrane stretches across the outer rows of hair- 1,1 j n.aud l.l i.spn t.xjiit o.spn c i> lg.sp Fig. 194.— Diagram of the organ of Corti (from Foster, after Retzius) : i.r, inner rod of Corti; o.r, outer rod of Corti; i.fte, inner hair-cell ; v.c, the group of nuclei beneath it; o.hc, outer hair-cells, or cells of Corti; C.I), the twin cells of Deiters (four rows) ; n.aud, the auditory nerve perforating the tympanic lip, l.l. and lost to view among the nuclei beneath the inner hair-cells: i.spn, the inner spiral strand <>f nerve- fibrils; t.spn, the spiral strand of the tunnel: o.spn, the outer spiral strand belonging to the tirst row of outer hair-cells ; the three succeeding spiral strands belonging to the three other rows are also shown ; nerve-fibrils are shown stretching radially across the tunnel^ //.<■, Etensen's cells; Cl.c, Claudius' cells; t.i. lymphatic epithelioid lining on the side toward the Bcala tympani : lg.sp, Ugamentum spirale; c, cells lining the spiral groove, overhung by the vestibular lip,l.v; m.t, tectorial membrane; a fragment, torn from it, remains attached to the organ of Corti just outside the outermost row of hair-cells. cells, the body <>f each of which is enclosed and is held at its top within a ring of the network (Fig. 195, d). Each of the cells of Deiters, described above, is continued upward in a process which is attached to a cross-bar or a ring of the reticulate membrane next outside its companion-cell of Corti. The inner or median line of the Deiters cell is also modified into a cuticular thread fused below to the basilar membrane and above to a ring of the reticulate membrane. Thus the audi- tory hair-cells of Corti may be regarded as suspended from the reticulate mem- brane, which in turn is supported by the cuticular processes of the cells of Deiters. which rest upon the basilar membrane, and by the phalangar pro- cesses of the rods of Corti. The physical contacl of the cells of Corti with those of Deiters is so intimat< — if, indeed, their substance is not continuous — that impulses generated in the one can probably easily be communicated to the other. The upper wall of the canalis cochlearis is made of a sheet of homogenous, fibrillated connective tissue covered with Hat cells, and stretches from the limbus of the spiral lamina outward and upward to the side wall of the THE SENSE OF HEARING. 379 coclilea. It is known as the membrane of Reissner. The periosteal con- nective tissne of the bony wall of the cochlea is generally well developed within the area enclosed between the membrane of Reissner and the membrana basilaris; it is particularly thick at the line of division between the scala media and the scala tympani, where it forms a projecting ridge at the outer attach- ment of the basilar membrane. This ridge is the spiral ligament; an exten- ..o.r.h Fig. 195.— Diagram of the constituents of the organ of Corti (from Foster, after Retzius) : a, inner hair cell; a', the head, seen from above; B, inner, b', outer, rod of Corti; pA, in each, is the phalangar pro- cess; c, the twin outer hair-cell ; Cc, the cell of Corti; h, its auditory hairs; n, its nucleus; x, Ilensen's body; D.c, cell of Deiters ; n' , its nucleus; ph.p, its phalangar process;/?/, the cuticular filament; m.b, basilar membrane; m.r, reticulate membrane; c', the head of a cell of Corti, seen from above; d, the organ of Corti, seen from above ; i.hc, the heads of the inner hair-cells ; i.r.h, the head and phalangar pro- ecu- <>f the inner rod ; o.r.li, the head of the outer rod, with ph.p, its phalangar process, covered to t! hand by the inner rods, but uncovered to the right; o.h.c, the heads of the cells of Corti, supported by the rings of the reticulate membrane; pli, one of the phalangse of tin' reticulate membrane. sion from it, gradually decreasing in thickness, reaches into both the vestibular and the tympanic scala. A thick layer of both columnar and cuboidaJ epithelium lines the con- nective tissue forming the outer wall of the canal is cochlearis. This epithe- lium is peculiar in that the blood-vessels of the underlying connective tissue penetrate between the epithelial cells themselves. The tectorial membrane (membrana tectoria) is a sheet of radially-fibrillated tissue, thin at its poinl of attachment to the vestibular lip of the limbus, and becoming thicker and then thinner again as it stretches out over the organ of Corti, reaching as tin- as the most external row of hair-cells. It is said to lie in actual contact with the rods of Corti and the free ends of the hair-cells, and it has been presumed to serve as a damper for the vibrations imparted to the organ of ( 'orti. 380 AN A 31 ERICA N TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. The researches <»t' Howard Avers' have led him to conclusions concerning the minute anatomy of the ear materially different from those just presented. Thus, Avers asserts that the so-called membrana tectoria is nothing more than the matted mass of hairs " which spring from the tops of the hair-cells and form a waving plume on the crest of the ridge of the organ of Corti." He also holds the membrana reticulata and several other structures described by different authors to be nothing more than artefacts produced by the methods of preserving and manipulating the specimens. According to Avers, the cochlear nerves end in the hair-cells and not freely between them, and they are probably continuous with the auditory hairs. Theory of Auditory Sensation. — It can hardly be doubted that the nervous structures of the cochlea form an organ of special sense for the per- ception of musical times and probably of noises as well. But no trustworthy conclusion can be maintained as to the precise mode of action of the auditory apparatus, due fact that the rods of Corti are absent from the cochlea? of birds, which evidently are capable of appreciating musical tones, shows that these structures may be accessory, but are not essential parts of the sensory apparatus. Starting from the fact that the basilar membrane splits readily in a radial direction, in which, moreover, it is tightly stretched between its attachments, Helmholtz2 long ago proposed the theory that the basilar membrane behaves toward vibrations reaching it like a series of stretched strings. As the wires of a piano have different rates of vibration according to their length, and respond sympathetically to correspondingly different notes sounded in their neighborhood, so it has been supposed that different radial fibres of the basilar membrane are set into sympathetic vibration by different rates of vibration in the fluids bathing them. These vibrations must be imparted to the structures in the organ of Corti, and the irritation of the nerves connected with the cells of Corti is a natural sequel. It may he repeated that, though the canal of the bony cochlea as a whole diminishes in diameter from base to cupola, the canal of the membranous cochlea, the scala media with its lower wall or basilar membrane, increases in diameter. Thus the radial fibres of the basilar membrane are longest near the apex of the cochela. The radial width of the basilar membrane, measured near the bottom, middle, and top. respectively, is given as 0.21 millimeter. 0.3 1 milli- meter, ami 0.36 millimeter. The waves of physical sound are thus supposed to he analyzed in the peripheral sense-organ, each auditory nerve-fibre excit- ing in consciousness a tone of a particular pitch, and the mind perceiving the simultaneous effects of different pendular vibrations as notes of different quality. 'Avers: Journal of Morphology, May, 1892. 2 Helmholtz : Toiienipjiii(/iut(/i-ii, 1*77, S. 210. THE SENSE OF HEARING. 381 0. The Relation between Physical and Physiological Sound. Production of Sound-waves. — Sound, in its physiological meaning, is a sensation which is the conscious appreciation of internal changes occurring in certain cells of the cerebral cortex. Fibres of the auditory nerve come into close relation with these cells, and in whatever way those fibres are excited the result is one and the same, a sensation of sound. The elaborate apparatus of the middle and internal ear is so constructed that the energy of mechanical oscillations in the external air is transmitted to the terminations of the auditory nerves in a manner to excite them. Sound, in a physical sense, consists in waves of alternate condensation and rarefaction travelling in the air from the point of origin of the sound, much as waves radiate over the surface of water from the point where a stone is dropped. Any sudden impulse, such as a puff of air, or the vibration of a solid body, as a stretched string or a tuning-fork, pushes the adjacent molecules of air against those further removed, and this impulse produces an area, or aerial shell, of increased density or condensation. The air being perfectly elastic, the molecules, relieved from pressure, spring back even beyond the position of equilibrium, and leave an area of decreased density or rarefaction. Thus a wave, consisting of a shell of condensation succeeded by a shell of corre- sponding rarefaction, moves through the air. This single air-wave is the simplest element of physical sound. When a number, no matter how great, of sound-waves simultaneously excite the same particle of air, the resultant motion of that particle is the algebraic sum of all the motions imparted to it by the single sound-waves considered separately. As any elastic body, when set vibrating, continues its oscillations for a time, so is it probable that strictly isolated air-waves do not occur. Any elastic body, such as a stretched string, or a tuning-fork, when set in vibration, sends out from itself a series of air- waves which succeed one another at a rate identical with the rate of vibration of the elastic body. Such a regular succession of air-waves striking upon the tympanic membrane sets the latter into correspondingly regular oscillations and produces in the auditory apparatus the sensation of musical torn. Loudness and Musical Pitch. — The more vigorous the vibrations of the oscillating body, the more forcibly arc the air-molecules which are struck by it driven forward ; and the greater their excursion or amplitude of movement, the greater is the force with which the tympanic membrane is driven inward when the moving air-wave strikes it. The loudness of the tone manifestly depends upon the extent of motion of the tympanic membrane, as doe- this on the amplitude of air-motion. Differed elastic bodies have different natural rates of oscillation. The more rapid the rate, the more frequent is the succes- sion of air-waves that strike upon the ear. It is said thai the apparent pitch of a tone is raised when its Intensity is lowered, and that such an elevation of pitch may equal one-tilth of a tone.1 Musical pitch is determined by the number of air-waves which pass a given point in a unit of time, or. in other words, by the rate of vibration of the sound-producing hotly. When 1 Broca : Jahresiberiehi der Physiologie, L897, 8. 111. 382 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. the vibration-rate increases the pitch is elevated, and vice versd. If some body capable of producing sound should have its rate of vibration changed grad- ually from 5 or 10 vibrations per second to 50,000 per second, no sensation of sound would be aroused until the vibrations reached the rate of about from 16 to 24 per second. The droning note of the 16-foot organ-pipe and the lowest bass of the piano represent a vibration-rate of 33 per second. In most persons sounds cease to be audible when the air-waves have a fre- quency of 16,000 per second, though to some the note produced by 40,000 vibrations is perceptible. It seems clear that some animals hear tones whose pitch is so elevated as to make them inaudible to human ears. When a mov- ing bell or whistle, as of a locomotive, rapidly approaches, its pitch seems to rise, and then to fall as it recedes. The reason for this variation is that the motion of the locomotive adds to or subtracts from the number of sound- waves reaching the ear iu a given time. In musical execution and in the ordinary uses of life the limits in the pitch of sounds are much narrower. Thus, as just stated, the lowest bass of the piano (C^ represents a vibration- rate of 33 in a second, while the highest treble (c'"") has that of 4224. As to the absolute number of vibrations necessary to produce the sensation of sound, it has been found that 2 or 3 vibrations excite the sensation of a mere stroke; 4 or 5 vibrations are necessary to give a tone; and some 20 or 40 are required to develop the full musical qualities of a tone.1 That is to sav, when a musical tone falls upon the ear its characteristics cannot be appreciated until 20 to 40 vibrations have been completed. Thus, from a physical scale representing aerial vibrations of indefinitelv various rapidity the mind selects and appreciates as sound a very small fraction. Tympanic Membrane as an Organ of Pressure-sense. — There is good reason to suppose that variations in air-pressure succeeding one another too slowly or too irregularly to produce sound-sensation are still of great import- ance in the extensive realm of sensations which but obscurely excite our con- sciousness. Slow inward movements of the tympanic membrane may still give rise to a perception of external changes. Thus, a blind man has been able to say correctly that he has passed by a fence, and whether it be of solid board or of open picket. If any one with closed eyes holds a book at half-arm's length in front of the ear, a different sensation will be experienced according as the book is turned flat or edgewise to the face; the feeling is one of "shut- in-ness" or "open-ncss," respectively. The air is in ceaseless agitation, and its waves, striking against various objects, must be reflected to the ear with an intensity dependent on the position and the physical character of the reflecting media. We may assert that the tympanic membrane is the peripheral organ of a pressure-sense by which we become more or less accurately aware of the nature and position of surrounding objects, irrespective of the sensations of sight and hearing. Whether that group of sensations depends on the excite- 1 Mach : Physikalischen Notizen Lotos, Aug., 1873; V. Krics und Auerbach: D>i Bois-Rey- moncTs Archiv fur Physiologie, 1877, S. 297; Helmholtz: Sensations of Tone, translated by Ellis. THE SENSE OF HEARING. 383 ment of tactile nerves in the tympanic membrane or of the auditory filaments in the internal ear is yet uncertain.1 Such sensations probably form an import- ant quota of that complex system of sensations which do not obtrude themselves on consciousness, but which, nevertheless, bring information from the outer world, and have an intimate association with the more or less reflex move- ments that preserve the equilibrium of the body. Overtones and Quality of Sound. — We have thus far considered only simple tones produced by simple vibrations of elastic bodies. Thus, a stretched string plucked at its middle vibrates throughout its whole length, the greatest amplitude of movement being at the middle point, which moves to and fro like a pendulum. It is very rare that a body set vibrating confines itself to a single pendular movement. Thus, a stretched string when struck not only moves as a single cord, but the string may break up, as it were, into two halves, each vibrating independently, but with twice the rate of movement of the whole length of string. Not only is this the case, but the string in its vibra- tion also breaks up into chords of one-third, one-fourth, one-fifth, etc. of its original length, giving rise to vibrations three, four, and five times as rapid as those produced by the whole string. In musical phrase, the middle c of the piano, when this key is struck, gives not only a note c representing 132 vibra- tions, but also its octave c' of 264 vibrations, the fifth above this of 396 vibrations, the second octave, 528, the third above this, 660, and so on. The vibration of a string, then, sends to the ear a complex series of tones each of which represents a simple pendular motion of the air. The lowest tone, that produced by the slowest rate of vibration of the string as a whole, is known as the fundamental tone. The pitch of the fundamental tone determines our estimate of the pitch of the whole complex note. The other tones produced by segmental vibration of the string are known as partial tones, upper partials, or overtones The fundamental tone is usually stronger than its accompanying overtones, the successively higher upper partials diminishing rapidly in intensity. Some musical instruments produce notes with a longer series of overtones than do others; the human voice is particularly rich in overtones. Instruments differ also in the greater or lesser strength and in the relative prominence of the individual overtones accompanying the fundamental. It is the number and the relative prominence of the overtones in a musical note that determine its quality. Thus, a violin, a cornet, and a piano, though sounding a note of the same pitch, would never be mistaken the one for the other; our discrimination of their notes depends simply upon the difference in the relative strength and the number of their overtones, the fundamental tone being the same throughout. The brilliancy and richness of musical notes is dependent on their wealth of upper partials. It is believed that a sound-producing body, like a stretched string, does not send to the ear a separate set of waves representing each of its segmental vibrations, but that all the waves aroused by it \\\>v together into a single series of waves of peculiar form. Such a composite wave may be 1 W. .lames: Psychology, 1890, vol ii. p. 140. 384 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. represented graphically by depicting under one another a series of waves having two, three, four, etc. times the rate of succession of the curve indicating the fundamental tone. W a vertical line be drawn across the series representing the vibration-rates of the various tones, and an algebraic addition be made of the distance of each point of intersection above or below the line of rest, the result will determine the position of the composite curve on the same vertical (Fig. 19G). It is evident that the form of the composite wave must change with every change in the number and relative prominence of musical overtones, and the movement imparted by it to the tympanic membrane and the wave B e Fro. 196.— The curve B represents twice the vibration-rate of a. When the two curves are combined by the algebraic addition of their ordinate*, the result is the periodic curve c (solid line , having a dif- ferent form , the dotted line of C is a reproduction of a. If b is displaced to the right until e falls under din a (change of phase), the combination of a and B will give the curve d, the dotted line in d repre- senting a as before. (After Helmholtz.) generated in the perilymph must have corresponding differences. Notes of different quality are produced by composite air- waves of different forms. But waves differing in form may still produce notes of the same quality ; for if, in the graphical figure, one or more of the curves representing simple tones be slid to the right or the left, the form of the composite wave will thereby be changed, but not the quality of the sound produced by it. In other words, change of phase of the partial tones does not alter t lie quality of the note.1 The quality of any complex note may be reproduced by sounding together a series of tuning-forks which have, respectively, the vibration-rate of the fundamental tone and that of one of the overtones of the complex note. Analysis of Composite Tones by the Ear. — According to the theory outlined on page 380, the composite wave, beating against the sensitive organ of the cochlea, is again analyzed into the elements composing it, one part of the basilar membrane vibrating sympathetically with one partial tone, another with another. The isolated irritation of each nerve-element arouses in the mind the idea of a tone of a certain pitch and loudness; but when a number 1 Belmholtz, op. ciL, pp. :!0-34. THE SENSE OF HEARING. ■>■> of such elements are simultaneously stimulated, the mind takes note, not of the individual sensations thereby aroused, but of a resultant sensation formed by the fusion of these. That apparently simple tones are actually made up of a number of partials, having rates of vibration which form simple multiples of the fundamental tone, may easily be demonstrated at the open piano. If any note, as c in the bass clef, be struck while the key of its octave c is depressed, and then the struck string be damped, it will be found that the octave c rings out with its proper note. So in turn the g above that, the second octave and the e above that, may be made to sound when the lower c is struck, because each of these strings is so tuned that its fundamental note has the same vibration-rate as one of the overtones of the lower c. A note sung near the piano may in the same way be analyzed more or less completely into its component tones. The organ of hearing certainly has some such power of musical analysis, for some cultivated ears can not only follow any special instrument in a play- ing orchestra, but can even distinguish the overtones in a single musical note. The ear has little or no power of distinguishing difference of pitch in tones of less than 40 or more than 4000 vibrations per second ; but in the upper median parts of the musical scale the sensitiveness to change of pitch is very acute. Thus, according to Preyer,1 in the double-accented octave a difference of pitch of one-half vibration in a second can be detected ; that is, in the octave included between 500 and 1000 vibrations per second, 1000 degrees of pitch can be perceived. Every elastic body is capable of sympathetic vibration ; that is, air- waves beating upon it at its own natural rate of vibration set it into corresponding motion. In the same manner a heavy pendulum may be forced into violent movement by exceedingly light taps with the finger, the only necessary condi- tion being that the impulses imparted by the finger be exactly timed to the periodic motion of the pendulum or to some multiple of it. A bodv capable of sympathetic vibration with some particular tone is set into vibration by that tone, and reinforces or magnifies it, whether the tone exists alone or as the fundamental of a complex note, or is contained in the latter simply as an upper partial. The analysis of musical sounds is usually carried out by the use of resona- tors, which are hollow cylinders or spheres of glass or of* metal, rather widely open at one pole, and narrow-pointed at the opposite end for insertion into the ear. The mass of enclosed air vibrates, according to its size and shape, at some particular rate, and it is very readily set into sympathetic vibration whenever its fundamental tone is contained in any sound reaching it. By this means it is possible strongly to magnify, and thus select, the individual over- tones contained in a note. The vowel sounds of human speech owe their difference of quality to the adjustment in size and shape of the resonant air- chambers above the vocal cords. 1 Veber die Grenzen der Tbnwahrnehmung, June, 1876. Vol. II.— 25 386 s\X AMERKWX TEXT- HOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. Inharmonic Overtones. — It will ho remembered that all the overtones con- tained in a musical note are produced by vibrations which are simple multiples of the rate of the fundamental tone. These overtones are properly called harmonic upper partials ; they are. according to Helmholtz, particularly charac- teristic of stretched strings and narrow organ-pipes. But most elastic bodies have proper tones which are not exact multiples of the fundamental, and which may be termed inharmonic upper partials. The high-pitched jingle heard when a tuning-fork is first struck represents the inharmonic upper par- tials of the fork. Stretched membranes have a great number of such inhar- monic overtones. Inharmonic upper partials, as might be expected, rapidly die out in a note of which they form a part. It is evident that inharmonic proper tones, when nearly of the same pitch, must interfere with one another and repress the development of a well-marked fundamental tone. Production of Beats. — When two tones of slightly different pitch are sounded together, the more rapid vibrations overtake the slower, so that at certain periods the crests, or phases of condensation, of two waves fall together, and the result is a phase of increased condensation and louder sound. The waves immediately cease to correspond, and diverge more and more until the crest of one falls upon the trough of another, the result being silence, or at least great diminution in the intensity of the sound. Such alternate augmenta- tion and diminution of the waves give rise to pulses in the sound, known technically as beats. This is one of the most familiar and important phenom- ena of musical art. If two tuning-forks on resonance-boxes vibrate in unison, a piece of wax >tuck to the prong of one fork will lower its tone and give rise to beats. The undulating sound caused by striking a bell or the rim of a thin glass tumbler is due to beats. When two notes not included in a perfect chord are sounded on the piano, beats are heard not only from the interference of the fundamental tones, but of the upper partials as well. It is the absence of beats in notes which should be in harmony, as those of the major chord, that deter- mines the instrument to be in tune. When two tones produce beats, the number of beats in a given time is equal to the difference between the number of vibrations involved in the two tones in the same time. For example, a tone produced by 256 vibrations in a second sounded with one of 228 vibrations would give 28 beats in a second. It is evident that the frequency of beats may be increased either by increasing the interval between the tones or by Striking tones of the same interval in a higher part of the scale. Beats which are not too frequent — from four to six in a second — have important musical value, but when they number thirty or forty in a second they become exceedingly dis- agreeable, irritating the ear in a manner analogous to the effect of a flickering light on the eve. When sufficiently Dear together the beats no longer produce an intermittent sensation. The number of beats in a second required to result in this fusion increases as we ascend the musical scale, varying from 16 beats at c of 64 vibrations per second to 136 beats at <■'" of 1024 vibrations.1 The reason for this variation lies in the progressive shortening of the waves as the 1 Mayer: Sound, 1891. THE SENSE OF HEARING. 387 sound becomes higher in pitch; for it is obvious that as we ascend the scale, and the waves of sound become progressively shorter, spaces would be left between the individual waves unless their number were proportionately increased. Harmony and Discord. — Tones are concordant, or harmonize, when they produce no beats on being sounded together ; they are discordant when beats are produced, and the painful sense of dissonance increases in intensity up to about 33 beats per second. Perfect concord is obtained by blending notes whose vibrations are to each other as small whole numbers. Thus, in the major cord c E G c the vibration-numbers are 132 165 198 264 their ratios are 4 5 6 8 If notes the ratios of whose vibration-rates can be represented only by large whole numbers are combined, a discord is formed, for the reason that their upper partials interfere with one another and cause beats ; there is no especial virtue in the small integer.1 Thus, in the discord c D e the vibration-numbers are 132 148.5 165 which are not reducible to small whole numbers.2 Combinational Tones. — When two tones are sounded together, there is produced a new, usually weaker, tone, whose vibration-number is the numerical difference between the vibration-rates of the original tones. It is therefore known as a differential tone. Such tones may arise from upper partials as well as from the fundamentals; they do not appear to be formed, as might be sup- posed, by the fusion of beats. Other "combinational " tones of more intricate relations, as well as beats, arise from the interaction of vibrations when many different notes, as those of an orchestra, are sounded together. To calculate the physical result of the combination of these impulses, which it is the duty of the tympanic membrane to transmit, is a problem of exceeding complexity. RisurnS. — To sum up the subject, musical sounds are distinguished in sen- sation by the three factors, loudness, pitch, and quality, sometimes called color or timbre. These sensations depend in turn on definite physical characters of air-waves: their amplitude, or the extent of motion of the air-molecules ; their frequency, or rate of succession of the waves; their form, which is deter- mined by the pitch and relative predominance of the upper partials combined with the fundamental tone. Fatigue. — That the ear is subject to fatigue toward a note that has been sounded is easily demonstrated in the following way: Strike a single Dote of, say, a major chord on the piano, and immediately afterward sound the full chord; the quality of the latter will be altered from its normal character, owing to the lessened prominence of the note which had been struck.3 We may therefore not improperly speak of a successive contrast in auditory sensa- 'Tyndall: Sound. 'Waller: Human Physiology, 1891. 3 Foster : Text-book of FVn/siology, 5th etl., 1891. 388 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. tions, analogous to visual successive contrast, by which our perception of every sound is colored by the sounds which have preceded it. Imperfections of the Ear. — Notwithstanding the mechanical provisions for making the external and middle ear a perfect transmitting apparatus, sound-perception is more or less modified by the action of the mechanism under certain conditions. Thus, Helmholtz believed that various combina- tional tones owe their origin chiefly to a periodic clicking in the joint between the malleus and mens bones. The resonance of the ear is a familiar fact, and through it high-pitched tones between e"" and g"" are reinforced and heard with undue loudness. Certain hissing sounds, the chirp of a cricket or the note of a locust, thus gain their intensity. This resonance probably is a feature of the external auditory meatus, since it is at once destroyed by apply- ing a small resonator to the ear (Helmholtz). Perception of Time Intervals. — The ear is eminently the sense apparatus for determining small intervals of time. Flashes of light succeeding each other at the rate of twenty-four in a second are fused in a continuous luminous impression by the eye, but by the ear at least one hundred and thirty-two audi- tory impulses as beats may be heard separately in a second. The power which the ear possesses of resolving complex air-waves into the host of pendular vibrations which may enter into their formation finds no analogy in the eye (Helmholtz). Musical Tones and Noises. — The important feature of the physical processes which give rise to musical tones is their periodicity. Every musical tone is produced by a regular succession of alternate rarefactions and condensa- tions in the air. The remaining class of sounds, known as noises, differs from musical sounds in the respect that such sounds are produced by an irregular succession of air-waves — one in which the interval between phases of conden- sation and rarefaction does not remain constant as in a musical note. Noises are for the most part made up of short musical notes so associated as not to "harmonize" with one another. As expressed by Helmholtz, the sensation of a musical tone is due to a rapid periodic motion of a sonorous body ; the sensation of a noise, to non-periodic motions. Functions of Different Parts of the Ear. — Concerning the functions of the different parts of the internal ear in their relation to sound-perception, it is generally believed, as previously stated, that the basilar membrane of the cochlea, with the nervous elements seated on it, is the organ concerned in the reception and transmission of musical sounds. There are a sufficient number of fibres in the basilar membrane to allow several to vibrate with every audible tone. It cannot, however, too strongly be impressed that no theory of physiolog- ical action should be accepted definitively without rigid experimental proof, and such evidence concerning the definite functions of the cochlea is almost wholly wanting. The sensory hair-cells on the macula? of the saccule and the utricle have been thought to have the duty of vibrating in response to any agitation imparted to the perilymph, without regard to its periodic character ; they THE SENSE OF HEARING. 389 might thus be termed sense organs for the perception of noises. Evidence will be adduced later (p. 407) for the belief that they are peripheral organs for the preservation of static equilibrium. The hair-cells on the crista? of the ampullae of the semicircular canals seem to have a special function in giving rise to sensations caused by changing the position of the head ; they thus are organs concerned with the preservation of the equilibrium of the body. Judgment of Direction and Distance. — The distance and direction from which sounds come to the ear are not perceived directly, but our estimate of them is a judgment based on the loudness and quality of the sound sensation, combined with a power of reasoning from past experience. Thus, in seeking to discover the direction whence a sound comes, it is usual for an observer to turn Fig. 197.-End-bulbs from human conjunctiva (from Quain, after Lonfrworth) : a, ramification of nerve- fibres in the mucous membrane, and their termination in end-bulbs, as Been with a Lens; B, end-bulb, highly magnified; a, nucleated capsule; b, core, the outlines of its component cells not seen; c, entering nerve-fibre branching, its two divisions to end in the bulb at d. the head to the position in which the sound is heard loudest, and thus to form an opinion as to the direction whence it comes. Errors of judgment as to the direction are frequent, owing to the sound reflected from some object appearing louder than that coming in a direct line from its source. It is said that when there is total deafness in one ear every sound seems to have Its origin on the side of the healthy ear. When the eyes are closed, sounds originating in the median plane of the head are very imperfectly localized, but tend to be projected upward, and somewhat in front, since this is the space from whirl) most sounds come to us.1 The quality as well as the loudness of a sound varies according to the distance of its source. Thus the lower tones die away earliest as a sound recedes, bringing the overtones into undue prominence. The art ot 1 Seashore: hoc cit. 390 AX AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. the ventriloquist consists largely in altering the quality of the sounds he pro- duces to imitate the quality they would naturally have if arising under the conditions which he would lead his hearers to believe to be their origin. A comparatively feeble sound near at hand may have the same quality as a loud one heard at a distance ; thus, a frog croaking in an adjoining room was once mistaken by the writer tor a large dog barking outside the building. D. Cutaneous and Muscular Sensations. General Importance of the Cutaneous and Muscular Sensations. — Cutaneous sensations are aroused by the operation of some form of energy on the skin, and they include the sensation- of touch, of temperature, and of pain. By muscular sensation is meant the ap- preciation which we have of the intensity and direction of muscular effort. Closely allied to this sensation is a general sensibility through which we gain a knowledge of the relative position of the parts of our bodies, irrespective of movements. The direction, size, distance, and surface features of external objects are usually made known to us through the sense of sight or of hearing. Yet these fundamental facts regarding the things about us do not become a part of knowledge through direct visual and auditory perception. Such knowledge is based on complex judgments concerning the meaning of auditory and visual phenomena ac- cording as they have, in past experience, been interpreted by tactile and muscular perceptions. That is, when reduced to its simplest terms, out- most practical and important knowledge of the world is the outgrowth of tactile and muscular perceptions ; by and with them all other sense- perceptions of objects have been corrected and compared. Thus, so simple a feat as the estimate of the size of a distant object is the result of Fig. 198. —Tactile corpuscle within a papilla of the skin of the hand (from Quain, after Ran- vier) : 71, n, two nerve-fibres pass- ing to the corpuscle; a, a, ter- minal varicose ramifications of the axis-cylinder within the cor- puscle. Pig. 191 hematic figure of a neuromuscular spindle of the first type, namely, with complex nerve-ending ; adult cat : '•.. capsule ; m. u.h.. motor nerve-bundle ; pi. < -. plate-ending; n. >>-.. nerve-trunk: ,,r. < .. primary ending; s. e., secondary ending ; b. to., axial muscle-fibres. 1 From Rufflni, Journal of l'i<e the heavier; and of two objects having the same size and weight, that which appears to be the smaller seems heavier.3 The simultaneous excitement of other sen- sations may modify that of pressure ; thus, when two coins of equal weight, 1 '' Tastsinn und Gemeingefiihl," Wagners Handworterbuch der Physiologic, 1846. 2 Quoted in Hermann's Handbuch der Physiologic, Bd. iii. '2, S. 336. 3 Dressier : American Journal of Psychology, 1894, vol. vi. No. 3. 394 AN AMERICAN TEXT- HOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. but one warm ami the other cold, are laid upon the hand or the forehead, the cold one appears to be much the heavier. There is a sensation of after-pressure depending for its strength on the amount of the weight and the length of time tins weight has been applied. In fact, this after-sensation may produce a striking effect on consciousness, a familiar example of which is the persistence of the sense of pressure of the hat-band alter the head-covering is removed. Even light weights leave an after-sensation, and, in order to be perceived as separate, must be applied at intervals of not less than -^^ to -§\-$ of a second. It is said that when the finger is applied to the rim of a rotating wheel provided with blunt teeth, the separate teeth are no longer felt, and the margin seems smooth, when the con- tacts succeed each other at the rate of 500 to 600 in a second.1 Vibrations of a string cease to be appreciated by the finger when they have a rate of between 1500 and 1600 per second. Tlie Localization of Touch-sensation. — When a touch-sensation is felt, the mind inevitably refers the irritation to some particular part of the surface of the body, and the sensation seems to be localized in this area. On the accurate localization of tactile sensations depends not only the safety of the individual, but also the performance of the ordinary acts of life. We may suppose that to each area of peripheral distribution of tactile nerve-fibres in the skin there corresponds an area of tactile nerve-cells in the brain. It can hardly be doubted that the nerve-cells are divided into physio- logical groups characterized by inherent and inborn quality-differences in the sensations aroused by their respective excitements. The reference of the sen- sations aroused by the excitement of definite nerve-cells to definite parts of the periphery is a power acquired through the physiological experiences of the earliest months of life. Through the sense of sight the seat of irritation is recognized, and through muscular sensation its relation to surrounding parts is experimentally explored, so that cumulative harmonious experiences of tactile, visual, and muscular sensations finally bring into correspondence the various areas with definite varieties of touch-sensation, or, to use an expression of Lotze's,2 every area of the skin acquires a " local sign " by which it is dis- tinguished in consciousness. This power of localization differs widely for different parts of the skin. The fineness of the localizing sense for any skin-area is easily estimated by determining how far apart the tips of a pair of compasses, applied to the skin, must be separated in order to be felt as two. For this experiment the compass- points musl be smooth, and they should not be applied heavily. The general result of such an inquiry is that the compass-points may be nearer together, and -till be distinguished as two, in proportion as the surfaces to which they are applied have greater mobility. Since it is just such parts of the body as the tips of the tongue and the fingers that are chiefly used in determining the position of objects, the advantage of such an arrangement is obvious. The 1 Landois and Stirling : Human Physiology, 1886. - Funke, in Hermann's Handbuch der Physiologic, Bd. iii. 2, S. 404. THE SENSE OF PRESSURE. 395 skin can thus be marked cut in areas (tactile areas), within each of which the compass-points are felt as a single object, but if they are separated so as to fall beyond the borders of these areas, they are at once perceived to be two. The following figures1 represent the distances at which the compass-points can just be distinguished as double when applied to various parts of the body: Tip of tongue • • 1.1 mm. Palm of last phalanx of finger 2.2 Palm of second phalanx of finger 4.4 Tip of nose 6.6 " Back of second phalanx of finger 11.1 Back of hand 29.8 " Forearm 39.6 Sternum 44 " Back 66 " It will be observed that accuracy of localization and sensitiveness to pressure find their most perfect manifestations in widely separate regions of the skin. Tactile areas are found to have a general oval form with the long axis parallel with the long axis of the member investigated. If the compass-points, separated, say, half an inch apart, be passed over the skin of the palm from the middle of the hand to the finger-tips, the sensation will be that of a single line gradually separating into two diverging lines. The result, of course, depends on the compass-points passing successively through areas of finer localization. If an area be marked out on a part of the skin where localiza- tion is poor, within which area two points simultaneously applied appear to be one, a single point moved within it is still perceived to change its place, and two points successively applied may be perceived to occupy different positions. The mental fusion or separation of the two compass-points, cannot depend altogether on their being placed over the terminal twigs of the same or of two adjoining nerve-fibres, for, were this the case, the points could be discriminated when separated by a very small distance across the line drawn between the endings of adjoining nerve-fibres, while on either side the points would have to be much more widely separated in the area of distribution of a single fibre. The important factor in the mental separation of two stimulated points is, that between such points there shall be found a certain number of sensory elements which are unstimulated.2 Practice in such experiments greatly increases the power to localize impressions. This improvement is evidently due not to the establishment of new nerves, bul to a more perfeel discrimination of sen- sations in the nerve-centres. Dressier" found thai after practice tor four weeks, the compass-points. which at die beginning had to be separated is millimeters on the skin of the forearm to be distinguished, could, at the end of the period, !»e recognized as two \\ hen only aboul I millimeters apart. A Imosl as great an improvement of localizing power was gained by the unexercised 1 Foster's Physiology, ">tli ed., 1891. 2 Weber: " Tastsinn und Gemeingefuhl," Wagner's Hamdworterbueh (!>;■ Physiologic, 1846. :1 Dressier : Loc. tit. 396 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. corresponding area of the skin of the opposite arm, but not by adjacent areas ; in other words, the localizing power is central, not peripheral. Practice aroused in both tactile areas a peculiar quality of sensation by which the area was recognized. The improvement in localizing power is gradually lost if unexercised. Pressure-points. — It has been found that if a light object, such as a lead- pencil, be allowed to rest by a narrow extremity successively on different parts of the skin, its weight will appear very different according to the part which is touched. If the spots on which the weight appears greatest be marked with ink, they will be found to have a constant position, and the skin may therefore be mapped out in areas of pressure-point*, which are believed to indicate the place of ending of pressure-nerve filaments. The pressure-points are rela- tively few in number and are principally collected about the hair-follicles. The Importance of the End-organ. — The sense of touch or pressure is a special sense ; that is, any irritation conveyed to the nerve-centres in which the nerves of pressure terminate gives rise to a feeling of touch, just as dis- turbance in the visual or the auditory centre is recognized in consciousness as a sensation of sight or of sound. The complex anatomical structures known as sense-organs may be considered as instruments each of which is differentiated in a manner to make it particularly irritable toward some special form of energy. Thus, the retina is most sensitive to the luminiferous ether; the organ of Corti, to waves of endolymph, etc. To this differentiation of structure the sensitiveness of the body to the forces of nature is chiefly due. The peripheral ending of the pressure nerve, whether a naked axis-cylinder or a touch-corpus- cle, is no doubt modified to be particularly irritable toward that form of energy manifested in the molecular vibration of the tissue solids, brought about by contact with foreign objects. Hairs, particularly those in certain localities of some animals, as the whiskers of the cat, appear to have the function of trans- mitting mechanical vibrations to the nerve-endings in greater intensity than could be accomplished through the skin alone. No true sense of touch is aroused by direct irritation of a nerve-trunk or exposed tissue, and touch-sensations do not arise from irritation of the internal surfaces of the body. A fluid of the temperature of the body gives, when swallowed, no sensation in the stomach ; when cooler or warmer than the body, there is a sensation due, probably, to a transmission of temperature change to the skin of the abdomen. Touch Illusions. — Certain peculiar errors in judgment may arise when tactile sensations are associated in a manner unusual in experience. Thus, in an experiment said to have been devised by Aristotle, if the forefinger and the middle finger be crossed, a marble rolled between their tips will appear to be two marbles; if the crossed finger-ends be applied to the tip of the nose, there seems to be two noses. The illusion is due to the fact that under ordinary circumstances simultaneous tactile sensations from the radial side of the forefinger and the ulnar side of the middle finger are always caused by THE SENSE OF TEMPERATURE. 397 two different objects. It is a not uncommon surgical operation to replace a loss of skin on the nose by cutting a flap in the skin of the forehead, without injury to the nerves, and sliding the flap round upon the nose. Touching the piece of transplanted skin gives the patient the sensation of being touched, not upon the nose, but upon the forehead; after a time, however, a new fund of experience is accumulated, and the sensation of contact with the transplanted flap is rightly referred to the nose. Persons who have suffered amputation of a lower limb often complain of cramps and other sensations in the lost toes. The illusion no doubt comes from irritation, in the nerve-stump, of fibres which previously bore irritations from the toes. 2. Temperature Sense. — The skin is also an organ for the detection of changes of temperature in the outer world. Such temperature differences prob- ably make themselves manifest by raising or lowering the temperature of the skin itself, and thus in someway irritating the terminal parts of certain sensory nerves, the temperature nerves. The sensitiveness of the skin to temperature variations is not the same in all parts; thus, it is more acute in the skin of the face than in that of the hand ; in the legs and the trunk the sensibility is least. We refer temperature sensations, somewhat like those of touch, to the periphery of the body, and localize them on the surface. The skin over various parts of the body may have different temperatures without exciting corresponding local differences of sensation. Thus, the forehead and the hand usually seem to be of the same temperature, but if the palm be laid upon the temples, there is commonly felt a decided sensation of temperature change in one or both surfaces. As in other sensations, fatigue and contrast play an important part in the sense perceptions of temperature, and stimuli of rapidly-changing intensity provoke the strongest sensations ; thus, when two fingers are both dipped into hot or cold water, the fluid seems hotter or colder to that finger which is alternately raised and lowered. In changing to a place of different temperature the skin for a time seems warmer or cooler, but soon the temperature sensation declines, and on return- ing to the original temperature the reverse feeling of cold or of warmth is experienced. For every part of the skin, then, there is a degree of tempera- ture, elevation above or depression below which arouses respectively the feeling of warmth or of cold, and the temperature of the skin determining the physiological null-point may vary within wide limits. The smallest differences of temperature that can be perceived fall, for most parts of the skin, within 1° C. The skin of the temples gives perception of differences of 0.4°-0.3° C. The surface of the arm discriminates 0.2°; the hollow of the hand, 0.5°-0.4° ; the middle of the back, 1.20.1 The size of the sensory surface affected modifies the intensity of temperature sensation : if the whole of one hand and a single finger of the other hand be dipped into warm or cold water, the temperature will seem higher or lower to the member having the greatest surface immersed. 1 Nothnagel : Deutsehes Archivfur klinische Medicin, L866, ii. S. 284. 398 AN AMERICAS TEXT-BOOK OF I'll Ysioijx ; V Fig. 201.— Cutaneous " cold " spots (vertical shading) and "hot" spots (horizontal shading), anterior sur- face uf the thigh (from Waller, after Goldscheider). Cold ami Warm Points. — The -kin is not uniformly sensitive to tem- perature changes, but its appreciation of them seems to be limited to certain points distributed more or less thickly over the sin face. These spots appear to be the places of termination of the temperature nerves in the epi- dermis ( Fig. 201 ). There is little doubt that there are two distinct varieties of temperature nerves, one of which appreciates elevation of temperature, or heat, and the other diminution of temperature, or cold. Thus, if a blunt-pointed metal rod be warmed and be touched in succession to various parts of the skin, at certain spots it will be felt as very warm, while at others it will not seem warm at all. If, on the contrary, the rod be cooled, a series of cold points may in the same way be made out. The point of an ordinary lead-pencil may be used with some success to pick out the cold spots. The " cold points " are more numerous than the " hot," and those of each variety are more or less distinctly grouped round centres, as would be expected from the manner of nerve-distribution, though the groups overlap to some extent (Fig. 201). Certain substances appear to act, prob- ably by chemical means, as specific excitants of the two sets of nerves. Thus, menthol applied to the skin gives a sensation of cold, while an atmo- sphere of carbon dioxide surrouuding an area of skin gives a sensation of warmth.1 The specific difference of the two sets of temperature nerves is indicated by the fact that when a warm and a cold body held close together are simulta- neously brought near the skin, the sensation is either one of both warmth and cold, or now one and now the other sensation predominates.2 Any stimulation, whether mechanical or electrical, applied to the sensitive points thus far de- scribed in the skin, for the appreciation of either pressure, heat, or cold, pro- vokes, when effective, only the proper sensation of that point; any irritation of a cold, hot, or pressure point gives rise, respectively, to the sensation of cold, heat, or pressure alone. As in other organs of special sense, the peripheral terminations of the temperature nerves seem modified to be especially irritable toward their appro- priate form of physical stimulus. Cold or heat directly applied to the nerve- trnnk excites no temperature sensation. Thus, if the elbow be dipped into a freezing mixture, as the lowered temperature penetrates to the ulnar nerve the sensation will be one, not of cold, but of dull pain, and it will be referred to 1 Goldscheider : Du Bois-ReymoncFa Archiv fur Physiologie, 1886, 1887 ; Blix: Zeitxchrift far :ie, 1884; Donaldson: Mind, 1885, vol. xxxix. 2 Czermak : Sitzwngsbt richtt )' the amount of motor energy sent out from the motor cells, or it may be due to the inflow of sensory impulses which show the tension to which the muscles have been subjected. The latter view has more to be said in its favor. 1 Brain, 1893-4. * S. J. Meltzer: Philadelphia Medical Journal, August 5, 1899, p. 12. 3 Brain, 1881. Vol. II.— 2G 402 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. Recent researches have demonstrated the existence of an abundant supply of sensory nerves, whose excitement must depend upon the exercise of skeletal muscles. Ciaccio1 has described the termination of sensory nerves in tendons as a splitting up of the nerve-fibres whose axis-cylinders, in the form of varicose threads, end freely as spirals or rings around the tendon- bundles. The joints seem to be particularly rich in sensory nerve-supply. Golgi - first described certain special modes of ending of sensory nerves just at the junction of the voluntary muscle with its tendon. This terminal organ is a fusiform corpuscle consisting of several delicate connective-tissue envelope.- with nuclei, and is situated on the surface of the tendon. One to several nerve-fibres enter each corpuscle, and, dividing and losing their medullary sheaths, break up into an arborization of naked axis-cylinders. The skeletal muscles themselves are extraordinarily rich in sensory nerve- supply. According to Sherrington,' " the proportion of afferent-fibres to total myelinate fibres ranges from a little more than one-third in some muscular nerves to a full half in others." These sensory fibres end, for the most part, in the so-called " muscle-spindles," which are fusiform bodies, usually just visible to the unaided eye (Fig. 199, p. 390). The spindles are for the most part scattered between the ordinary muscle-fibres, though many abut upon intramuscular septa or are in the immediate vicinity of aponeuroses. As many as thirteen spindles have been counted in one cross-section of thegenio- glossus muscle. Sherrington ' calculates that the number of spindle-organs is sufficient to account for nearly or quite two-thirds of all the afferent fibres demonstrated to exist in the nerve-trunks of the limb muscles. It is worth observing that the spindle-organs have not been demonstrated in the eve muscles nor in the intrinsic muscles of the tongue. The muscle-spindle con- sists of a central core of modified muscle-fibres inclosed in an outer capsule formed of several layers of concentrically disposed membranous lamellae composed of connective tissue. Between the capsule and the central muscle- bundle is a wide lymph-space traversed by a network of delicate filaments. In forming the spindle two or three ordinary muscle-fibres of the red variety become invested at the proximal end of the organ by a definite sheath of connective tissue. As they penetrate further into this envelope the muscle- fibres tend to split lengthways, each fibre giving rise to perhaps three " daughter "-fibres, which are proportionally of less diameter. The striatum and fibrillation are frequently confined to the outer portion of these daughter- fibres, some of which are devoid of sarcolemma. For the middle third of its course in the muscle-spindle each daughter-fibre bei es thickly crusted with a sheet of nuclei. Toward the distal end of the spindle the muscle- fibres often merge in tendon-bundles, which finally combine with the fibrous tissue forming the capsule of the spindle ; so that of the two ends of the axial bundle within the spindle, one is muscular and the otheris tendinous. According to Rufrini/' sensory nerves may end upon the axial muscle- 1 Barker: The Nervous System, 1899, |>. 105 - Ibid. 3 Sherrington : Journal of Physiology, L895, vol. xvii. p. 229. 4 Ibid. 5 Ruffini : Ibid., 1898, sxiii. 190. MUSCULAR SENSATION. 403 fibres of the spindle in either or all of three different modes (Fig. 199): 1. The axis-cylinder may flatten out and twine in rings and spirals about the muscle-fibre. 2. The axis-cylinder may break nj> into a number of leaflets applied to the muscle-fibre (secondary mode). :]. The axis-cylinder may end in a plate of varicose fibrils resembling the motor end-plate. When we consider that it is through muscular sensation that we derive our most accurate conceptions of the form, weight, and position of objects, and through which we explore our own body-surface and distinguish its areas of localization ; that this is the fundamental sense by which the sensations arising in most other organs are tested and verified ; and that it is from the sense of muscular movement that we can form ideas of time and space, — it may well be regarded as the mother of all sense-perceptions. Normal muscles, even when function- ally inactive, are still in a state of tonic contraction ; it is not improbable that this tone is a reflex action whose sensory element is formed by the impulses travelling along nerves of muscular sensation. Such impulses are probably indispensable to the preservation of the equilibrium of the body. Sherrington found that if he separated the aponeurosis belonging to the distal portion of the vastus medialis muscle, under which the muscle-spindle- are numerous, the knee-jerk could no longer be excited through the muscle. Our appreciation of the weight of bodies is determined by lilting them. But even in so simple an exercise of the muscular sense as this the judgment is subject to extraordinary illusions depending on the preconception of the weight of a body, and consequent muscular effort put forth in lifting it. When bodies having the same weight and size, such as appropriately loaded pieces of iron, cork, and wood, are compared, the specifically lighter body will seem to be heavier. "Before lifting an object we normally estimate the approximate weight by sight, and the effort to be exerted in lifting is adjusted semi-automatically upon the basis of this preliminary estimate. If insufficient effort is put forth at the beginning of the lifting, the weight of the object will be overestimated. If too great effort is put forth, the weight of the object will be underestimated."1 In comparing the weight of objects having different sizes the illusion takes another direction. Thus an inflated paper bag may be estimated to have the same weight as a piece of lead weighing sixty times as much." The clinical study of disease in the central nervous system affords strong evidence of the functional independence of the sense organs involved in the appreciation of touch, heat, cold, and pain. In certain diseases of the spinal cord, areas of skin may be mapped out in which sensations of pressure are lost, but those of temperature remain, and vice versd. In other diseases the patient can appreciate warmth applied to the skin, but not cold. The sensations of cold and pressure seem to be usually lost or retained together, while those of warmth and pain have a similar connection. It is a peculiar fact that sometimes in the early stages of ether and chloroform narco- sis the sense of touch remains while that of pain is abolished. Funke3 refers 'Seashore: Op. rit. 'Wolfe: Paycholoc/iml Rivim; ISDN, p. 25. 8 " Der Tastsinn," Hermann's Handbuch der Phyaiologie, Bd iii. 8. 2. 404 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. to two cases in which, while the tactile sense was preserved, muscular sensation was lost, and an object could be held in the grasp only while the eyes were turned upon it. Hunger and Thirst. — Hunger and thirst are peculiar sensations which depend partly on local and partly on general causes. Diminution in the bulk of water and of circulating aliment in the body no doubt causes excitement of sensory nerves on which depend the feelings of thirst and hunger, but in ordinary life these feelings are dependent on the physical condition of certain mucous surfaces. Any circumstance which causes drying of the lining mem- brane of the mouth provokes thirst, and some condition of the empty stomach arouses hunger. Thirst may be assuaged by introducing water directly into the stomach through a gastric fistula, though to effect the purpose a larger quantity must be employed in this way than by the mouth. Hunger in a somewhat similar manner may be appeased by rectal alimentation. It seems probable, however, that these sensations as usually felt are the result of a sort of habit, depending on the physiological condition of the secreting and absorbing mechanisms of the alimentary canal. Clinical observation has shown that " bulimia," or voracious appetite, is frequently a result of disease in certain parts of the central nervous system. We are therefore justified in speaking of a " hunger-centre." ' E. The Equilibrium of the Body; the Function of the Semicircular Canals. The term equilibrium, as applied to the condition of the body, whether at rest or in motion, indicates a state in which all the skeletal muscles are under control of nerve-centres, so that they combine, when required, to resist the effect of gravity or to execute some co-ordinated motion. The preservation of equilibrium is manifestly of fundamental importance in animal life, and we find, accordingly, several mechanisms sharing in this function. That the motor co-ordinating centres may act properly, they must receive sensory impres- sions conveying information of the relative position of the body at any given moment. The sum-total of these sensations may be characterized as the sense of equUibrivm, and it is probably not going too far to assume that every known sensation contributes to this fund of information. Thus, in ordinary life the position of objects is commonly determined by the sense of sight: when one tries to walk while looking through a prism, objects are not properly localized by vision, and improper co-ordination results. The contact of the soles of the feet with the ground, and that of the surface of the body with various objects, are common sources of information as to our relation with the environment. Standing upright, and still more when in motion, the muscular sense is active in appreciating the tension, active or passive, of the muscles. In the erect position, with eves closed, a writing point attached to the head will show that the body sways in a peculiar manner indicating successive contraction of differ- ent groups of muscles; and a person with failure of muscular and tactile sen- 1 Ewald : Disease a of the Stomach, p. 397. THE SENSE OF EQUILIBRIUM. 405 sibility, as in locomotor ataxy, cannot stand with eyes closed, and his move- ments, even when sight is employed, are exaggerated and unnatural. Attention has previously been called to the fact that air-waves, irrespective of those producing sound-sensations, exert an influence upon the tympanic membrane by which we are capable of appreciating the presence and, to some extent, the physical character of objects. Whether this sensation involves the nerves of touch, those of common sensibility, or those distributed to the internal ear, is uncertain. In the absence of any of these sensations the loss may be made up by more perfect development of others. Ordinarily, the sensory information from all these sources, when compared in consciousness, harmonizes and gives rise to a concrete idea of position. Frequently, however, one of the sources of sense- impression suddenly fails us or its testimony conflicts with that of other sense organs; the result is disturbance of equilibrium. A very common outcome of this conflict of sensations is dizziness or nausea. The distress arising from wearing ill-fitting glasses and the sensations experienced when one looks down from a high eminence are examples in point. Internal disorders exciting nerves of common sensation have the same effect, though the relation borne by visceral sensations to equilibrium is very ill known. A false idea of position of the body, a sense of falling in one direction or another, may lead to sudden effort of recovery by which the person is precipitated to the opposite side. Thus, when looking at rapidly-moving water erroneous ideas of equilibrium are gained through the visual sense, and there is a strong tendency for the body to precipitate itself in one direction or another. When, in going up a stair- case, one miscalculates the number of steps, a peculiar sensation of want of equilibrium is aroused through the muscular sense. It is clear, then, that the sense of equilibrium is served by various sense organs, and a complete discussion of this function would entail a consideration of the whole field of nerve-muscle physiology. There is, however, good reason for believing that there is a special sense organ for determining the position and direction of movement of the head and, by inference, of the whole body. The terminal organ of this sense apparatus of equilibrium is found in the system of semi- circular canals of the internal ear. Experiments on the lower animals, chiefly performed on birds, show a con- stant motor disturbance to follow division of any or all of the semicircular canals. These disturbances are of two kinds. When the animal is at rest it does not stand in a natural fashion, but sprawls in a more or less exaggerated degree. It holds its head in an unnatural position, as with the vertex touch- ing the back, or with the beak turned down toward the legs or benl over to one side. Immediately after the operation, and whenever it is disturbed, the animal goes through peculiar forced movements, together with rolling or twitching of the eves, of various kinds and degrees of violence, depending on the position and number of canals severed. The disturbance varies from simple unsteadiness in gait, with swaying motions of the head, to complete lack of co-ordination and a violence of movement almost comparable to that 406 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF I'll VSIOLOGY. of a chicken whose head lias been cut off. Essentially the same results have been determined to follow injury of the semicircular canals of widely different groups of animals. These results have l>c• the semicircu- un(]er observation feels a sensation of motion lar canals (after Waller): s, superior p canal ; p, posterior canal ; h, horizontal in the opposite direction. Each of tliese re- canaL suits should be expected to follow were the theory in question correct. The observations of James have shown that with deaf mutes in whom the internal ear was at fault rapid rotation in an ordinary " swing " failed to produce the dizziness which is the common effect in ordinary individuals. On the other hand, diseases which may be sup- posed to alter the intra-labyrinthine pressure are characterized by the symp- toms of vertigo and inco-ordination of movement. The presumable effect of cutting the semicircular canals is that the escape of endolymph changes the pressure upon the sensory hair-cells and gives the animal the sensation of falling in one direction or another, so that he is impelled to make compensa- tory or forced movements to counteract this imaginary change of position. In birds and in fishes, whose life is passed more or less exclusively in a medium in which tactile and muscular sensation can contribute little to the sense of equilibrium, the semicircular canals are especially well developed.1 In fishes, though section of the canals themselves produces no disturbance, division of 1 Bewail: Journal of Physiology, 1884, iv. p. 339. THE SENSE OF EQUILIBRIUM. 407 the nerves Supplying the ampullae usually gives rise to marked forced move- ments, as shown in somersaults, spiral swimming, etc., when set free in the water. When, however, the nerves are cut with great eare, with sharp scis- sors, so as to avoid traction on or crushing of the nerves, such forced move- ments do not follow. Lee1 found that when a fish is turned in different positions there is a compensatory change in the direction of the fins and the optic axes determined by the semicircular canal in whose plane the movement is made. He con- cludes that "Each canal has a principal and a subordinate function. The former is the appreciation of rotational body movements in its own plane and toward its side of the body ; the latter is the appreciation of similar move- ments, but in the opposite direction." Electric stimulation of the ampullary nerves or mechanical pressure upon the ampulla? excites equally definite movements of eyes and fins, and the ocular result of nerve-irritation is the exact opposite of that of nerve-section. The difference in function between the divisions of the internal ear is indicated by investigations on albinos. White animals with blue eyes are deaf, but possess the normal power of equilibration. Rawitz2 found the cochlea in such creatures to be much reduced and the organ of Corti atro- phied, while the semicircular canals were normal. According to Lee 3 and others, the equilibrium of rest and motion, or static and dynamic equilibrium, depends upon the irritation of different nerve-ter- minals. The manner of action of the latter has been considered. As to the nervous mechanism on which static equilibrium depends, Lee is of the opinion that the knowledge of the position of the head while at rest comes from the rela- tion of the otoliths in the vestibular sacs to the nerve-endings on the maoulce acustlcce. These otoliths form considerable masses in the ears of fishes, and the intensity and direction of their pressure upon hair-cells must vary with the spatial relations of the head, and thus be comparable, in the sense of posi- tion which they arouse, to the tactile sensations derived from the soles of the feet in man. The opinion may be ventured that in the semicircular canals we have a sense-organ of a peculiar kind. The evidence is satisfactory that impulses generated in the nerves of the ampulla?, and probably of the vestibular sacs also, give rise to sensations of position both dynamic and static And it is highly probable that such sensations form a constant basis for our notion of the spatial relations of the head. lint the preservation of equilibrium does not depend wholly upon the special sense-organ, as does sight upon the eye. For the muscular and tactile, not to speak of the visual and other senses, supply information in the same direction, and, no doubt, these may to a cer- tain extent vicariously fill the function of the semicircular apparatus when this is abolished. 1 Lee: Journal of Physiology, xv. p. :;il ; xvi. p. 192. 2 Rawitz : Zoologiseher Jahresbericht, 1896. 8 Journal of Physiology, xv. p. 31 1, xvi. p. 192. 408 AN AMERICA X TEXT-BOOK OE PHYSIOLOGY. F. Smell. The complex paired cavity of the nose is divisible into a lower respiratory and an upper olfactory tract, the mucous membrane over each of which is distinctive. The covering of the respiratory tract is known as the Schneider- ian or pituitary membrane; its surface is overlaid with cylindrical ciliated epithelium, the ciliary current of which is directed posteriorly toward the pharynx. The Schneiderian membrane lines the lower two-thirds of the septum, the middle and inferior turbinated bodies, and the bony sinuses which communi- cate with the nasal chamber. The mem- brane upon the turbinated bodies and the lower part of the septum is composed largely of erectile tissue. The function of the respiratory tract is threefold : it restrains the passage of solid particles into the lungs; it warms the air inspired to approximately Fn.i. 203. -Section of olfactory mucous mem- brane (after V. Brunn) : the olfactory cells are in black. Fig. 204.— Cells of the olfactory region (after V. Brunn): a, olfactory cells; 6, epithelial cells; n, central process prolonged as an olfactory nerve- lilpril; /. uucleua; c, knob-like clear termination of peripheral process ; //, bunch of olfactory hairs. the temperature of the body ; and it gives up moisture sufficient nearly to saturate the air. The olfactory mucous membrane, which alone is the peripheral organ for smell, is seated in the upper part of the nasal chamber, away from the line of the direct current of inspired air. The membrane is thick and is covered by an epithelium composed of two kinds of cells, columnar and rod cells. The latter are the true olfactory cells (Figs. 203, 204), with which the fibres of the olfactory nerve are known to be connected. These olfactory cells, in fact, are comparable to nerve-cells in that the fibres connected with them, the fibres composing the olfactory nerve, are direct outgrowths from the cells ( Fig. 205), essentially similar in every way to the nerve-fibre processes springing from nerve-cells in the nerve-centres. In this respect the olfactory cells differ from the sensory cells in other organs of special sense. The membrane THE SEWSE OF .SMELL. 409 appears to be not ciliated except near its juncture with the Sclineiderian membrane, where the columnar cells acquire cilia and gradually pass over into the cells covering the respiratory tract. Substances exciting the sense of smell exist as gases or in a fine state of division in the air inspired. They reach the olfactory mucous membrane by diffusion, assisted by the modified inspiratory movements of " sniffing " and " smelling," and are most acutely perceived when the air containing them is warmed to the body-temperature. The amount of odoriferous matter that may thus be recognized is extraordinarily small; thus, it is said that in one liter of air the odor of 0.000,005 gram of musk and of 0.000,000,005 gram of oil of peppermint can be perceived.1 The odoriferous particles probably excite the Fig. 205— Diagram of the connections of cells and fibres in the olfactory bulb (Schiifer, in Quoin's Avat- omy) : otf.c, cells of the olfactory mucous membrane ; olf.n, deepest layer of the bulb, composed of the olfactory nerve-fibres which are prolonged from the olfactory cells; gl, olfactory glomeruli, containing arborization of the olfactory nerve-fibres and of the dendrons of the mitral cells; ro.c, mitral cells; a, thin axis-cylinder process passing toward the nerve-fibre layer, n.tr, of the bulb to become continuous with fibres of the olfactory tract; these axis-cylinder processes are seen to give off collaterals, some of which pass again into the deeper layers of the bulb; «', a nerve-fibre from the olfactory tract ramifying in the gray matter of the bulb. sense of smell by coming into contact with the olfactory epithelium after solu- tion in the layer of moisture covering it. This epithelium is easily thrown out of function, as the common loss of smell when there is a "cold in the head " testifies. When the nostril is filled "with water in which an odorous substance is dissolved, no sensation of smell is excited, but it is said thai if normal salt-solution, which injures the living tissues less than water, be used as the solvent, the odor can still be perceived. In many lower animals the sense of smell has an acuteness and an importance in their economy unknown in the human race. It is probable that not only do differenl races have their distinctive odors, but that each individual exhales an odor peculiar to himself, distinguishable by the olfactory organs of certain animals. The classification 1 Passy : Comptes-rendus de la Sodete de Biologic, 1892, p. 84. 410 AN AMERICAN TEX1-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. of odors is not very definite, and the relation of odors to one another in the way of contrast and harmony is ill understood. No limited number of pri- mary sensations, as in vision, have been discovered out of which other sen- sations can be composed. Certain sensations, as those due to the inhalation of ammonia and other irritant gases, are thought to be due to excitement of the nasal filaments of the fifth nerve, and not of the olfactory. Subjective sensations of smell are sometimes experienced, the result of some irritation arising in the olfactory apparatus itself. Finally, in man sensations of smell have their most important uses in con- nection with taste; many so-called "tastes" owe their character wholly or partly to the unconscious excitement of the sense of smell. G. Taste. The peripheral surfaces concerned in taste include, in variable degree, the upper surface and sides of the tongue and the anterior surfaces of the soft palate and of the anterior pillars of the fauces. Other parts of the buccal and pharyngeal cavities are, in most persons, devoid of taste.1 The chief peripheral sensory organs of taste are groups of modified epi- thelial cells, known as taster-buds (Fig. 206), seated in certain papillae of the tasting surfaces. According to some authors, only parts provided with taste- buds can give taste-sensations.2 The structure of taste-buds is most easily studied in the papilla foliata of the rabbit, a patch of fine, parallel wrinkles found on each side of the back part of the tongue of the animal. The taste-bud is a somewhat globular body seated in the folds of mucous membrane between the furrows of the papilla. It is made up of a sheath of flattened, fusiform cells enclosing a number of rod-like cells each of which terminates in a hair-like process. These cells surround a central pore which opens into a furrow of the papilla. The hair-bearing cells recall the appearance of the olfactory rod-cells, and are probably the true sensory cells of taste, since between them terminate the filaments of the gustatory nerve. In the human tongue taste-buds are con- fined to the fungiform papillae, seen often as red dots scattered over the upper surface; to the circumvallate papillae, the pores of the buds opening into the groove around the papilla; and to an area just in front of the anterior pillar of the fauces, which somewhat resembles the papilla foliata of the rabbit. The sensory nerves distributed to the tongue include filaments from the glosso-pharyngeal, the lingual branch of the fifth, and the chorda tympani. The relation of these nerves to the sense of taste has been the occasion of much dispute. The weight of evidence probably favors the belief that the glosso-pharyngeal is the nerve of taste for the posterior third of the tongue, while the lingual and, to some extent, the chorda carry taste-impressions from the anterior two-thirds. Clinical cases have been cited to show that all the 1 V. Vintechgau : " Geruchsinn," Hermann's Handbnch der Physiologie, iii. 2, 1880. 2Cameror: Zeiischrifl fur Biologie, 1870, vi. 8. 440; Wilczynsky : Hofmann und Schwalbe's Jahresbericht der Physiol., 1875. THE SENSE OF TASTE. 411 WHSSmm ■%0mmmmtm Fig. 206.— Section through one of the taste-buds of the papilla foliata of the rabbit (from Quain, after Ranvier), highly magnified: p, gustatory pore ; «, gustatory cell ; r, sustentacular cell ; in, leucocyte containing granules; e, superficial epi- thelial cells ; a, nerve-fibres. gustatory fibres arise from the brain as part of the glossopharyngeal nerve, whatever may be their subsequent course to the tongue. On the contrary, other cases have shown a marked loss of taste-sensation following upon lesions of the fifth nerve at or near its origin from the brain, while still others indi- cate that some of the taste-fibres may arise in the seventh nerve. The point is of practical importance in diagnosis, in the interpretation of loss of taste over any given part of the tongue, but the contradiction in the clinical cases reported has led to the general belief that the origin and course of the gusta- tory fibres are subject to considerable individual variations. Our taste-perceptions are ordinarily much modified by simultaneous olfac- tory sensations, as may easily be dem- onstrated by the difficulty experienced in distinguishing by taste an apple, an onion, and a potato, when the nostrils are closed. In the condition of anosmia the ability to discriminate between tastes is much below par. Sight has also an important influence, at least in quickening the expectancy for individual flavors. Every smoker knows the blunting of his perception for burning tobacco while in the dark ; various dishes having distinctive flavors are said to lose much of their gustatory characteristics when the eves are bandaged.1 The intensity of gustatory sensation increases with the area to which the tasted substance is applied. The movements of mastication are peculiarly adapted to bring out the full taste-value of substances taken into the mouth, and the act of swallowing, by which the morsel is rubbed between the tongue and the palate, has been proved to develop tastes not appreciable by simple contact with the sensory surface. A considerable area in the mid-dorsum of the tongue is said to be devoid of all taste-sensibility.2 The sensitiveness of taste-sensation is greatest when the exciting substance is at the temperature of the body. Weber3 found that when the tongue was dipped during one-half to one minute in water either at the freezing tempera- ture or warmed to 50° C, the sweet taste of sugar could no longer be appre- ciated by it. It is probable that sapid substances reach the sensory endings of the nerves of taste only after being dissolved in the natural fluids of the mouth, and any artificial drying of the buccal surfaces or alteration of their secretion must affect taste-perceptions. 1 Cf. Patrick: "Studies in Psychology," Univ. Iowa, 1899, vol. ii. 'Shore: Journal of Physiology, 1892, vol. xiii. p. 191. 3 Archivfiir Anaiomie wnd Physiologie, is 17, S. 'M2. U2 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF I'll YSIOIJ Hi Y The excitement of the taste-nerves appears to depend not so much on the absolute amount of the substance to be detected as on the concentration of the solution containing it. Thus, when, 1 part of common salt to 213 of water was tasted by Valentin,1 11 cubic centimeters of the fluid was sufficient to give a saltish taste; when diluted so that the ratio of salt to water was 1 to 426, 12 cubic centimeters taken in the mouth scarcely gave the salt taste. Sulphate of quinine dissolved in the proportion 1 to 33,000 gave a decided bitter taste, but a solution 1 to 1,000,000 was with difficulty perceived as bitter. It has generally been conceded that all gustatory sensations may be built up out of four primary taste-sensations — namely, bitter, sweet, sour, and salt. Some authors even limit the list to tastes of bitter and sweet (V. Vintschgau). A uditory. Gustatory. Tactile. PIG. 207-Diagram showing the mode of termination of sensory nerve-fibres in the auditory, gustatory, and tactile structures of vertebrata (from Quain, after Retzius). Each sense organ may be considered as essentially constructed of a nerve-cell with two processes, one finding its way centrally to cluster round other nerve-cells or their processes, and the other to terminate in the periphery. In the organ of smell the peripheral process is very short and is directly irritated by foreign particles, the original nerve-cell being represented by the olfactory cell (Fig. 291). In the organs of touch the nerve-cell is found in the ganglion of the posterior spinal nerve-root; the peripheral process is very long Mini is acted on indirectly through the modified epithelium round which it clusters. The same may be said of the other sense organs. See Quain's Anatomy, 10th ed., vol. iii. pt. 3, p. 152. There is strong reason to believe that corresponding to the four primary taste- sensations there are separate centres and nerve-fibres, each of which, when excited, gives rise only to its appropriate taste-sensation. Substances which arouse the sense of taste are not appreciated in uniform degree over the surface of the tongue. Thus, to V. Vintschgau, at the tip of the tongue acids were perceived acutely, sweets somewhat less plainly, and bitter substances hardly at all. It is generally admitted that sweet and sour tastes are recognized chiefly at the front, and bitter, together with alkaline tastes, by the posterior 1 Lehrbuch der Physiohgw, 1848. THE SENSE OF TASTE. 413 part of the tongue. Strong evidence in favor of the specific difference between various taste-nerves is found in the fact that the same substance may excite a different gustatory sensation according as it is applied to the front or the back of the tongue. Thus, it has been demonstrated that a certain compound of saccharin (para-brom-benzoic sulphimide) appears to most persons to be sweet when applied to the tip of the tongue, but bitter in the region of the circum- vallate papillae.1 Oehrwall 2 has examined the different fungiform papillae scattered over the tongue with reference to their sensitiveness to taste-stimuli. One hundred and twenty-five separate papillae were tested with succinic acid, quinine, and sugar. Twenty-seven of the papillae gave no response at all, indicating that they were devoid of taste-fibres. Of the remaining ninety-eight, twelve reacted to suc- cinic acid alone, three to sugar alone, while none were found which Mere acted upon by quinine alone. The fact that some papillae responded with only one form of taste-sensation is again evidence in favor of the view that there are separate nerve-fibres and endings for each fundamental sensation ; but the figures given in the experiments show that the majority of the papillae are provided with more than one variety of taste-fibre. An extract of the leaves of a tropical plant, Gymnema silvestre, when applied to the tongue, renders it incapable of distinguishing the taste of sweet and bitter substances ; it probably paralyzes the nerves of sweet and bitter sensations. When a solution of cocaine in sufficient strength is painted on the tongue, the various sensations from this member are said to be abolished in the following order: (1) General feeling and pain; (2) bitter taste; (3) sweet taste ; (4) salt taste ; (5) acid taste ; (6) tactile perception (Shore). That there are laws of contrast in taste-sensation has long been empirically known. Thus, the taste of cheese enhances the flavor of wine, but sweets impair it (Joh. Miillcr). It is unfortunate, from a hygienic standpoint at least, that in this most important department of the physiology of sensation investigations are almost wholly wanting. Certain tastes may disguise others without physically neutralizing them ; when, for example, sugar is mixed with vinegar, the overcoming of the acid taste is probably effected in the central nerve-organ.3 1 Howell and Kastle: Studies from the Biological Laboratory of Johns Hopkins University. 1887, iv. 13. 2 Skandinavisches Archil) fitr Physwlogie, 1890, vol. ii. S. 1. 3 Briicke: Vorlemngen iiber Physiologie, 1876. IV. PHYSIOLOGY OF SPECIAL MUSCULAR MECHANISMS. A. The Action op Locomotor Mechanisms. The Articulations. — The form, posture, and movements of vertebrates are largely determined by the structure of the skeleton and the method of union of the bones of which it is composed. There are two hundred bones in the human skeleton, and they are so connected together as to be immovable, or to allow of many varieties and degrees of motion. There are four prin- cipal methods of articulation : 1. Union by Bony Substance (Suture). — This form of union occurs between the bones of the skull. These bones, which at birth are independent structures connected by fibrous tissue, gradually grow together and make a continuous whole, only a more or less distinct seam remaining as witness of the original condition. 2. Union by Fibro-Cartilages (Symphysis). — The bodies of the verte- bra' and the sacro-iliac and pubic bones are closely bound together by disks of fibro-cartilage. This material, which is very strong, but yielding and elastic, acts as a butler to deaden the effect of jars, permits of a slight amount of movement when the force applied is considerable, and restores the bones to their original position on the removal of the force. The spinal column can be thought of as an elastic staff; the capacity for movement differs greatly in different regions, however, partly on account of differences in the thickness of the intervertebral disks as compared with the antero- posterior and lateral diameters of the bodies of the vertebrae, and more espe- cially on account of the method of contact of the superior and inferior verte- bral processes. In the cervical region the disks are thick and the diameter of the vertebras is small, and this permits of considerable bending in all directions and a certain amount of rotation. In the dorsal region a slight amount of bending from side to side and a slight amount of rotation are pos- sible ; but backward bending is inhibited by contact of the articular processes, and forward bending is prevented by the strong articular ligaments. In the lumbar region bending in all directions is more free, but rotation is made impossible by the interlocking of the articular processes.1 3. Union of Fibrous Bands (Syndesmosis). — Some of the bones, as those of the carpus and tarsus, are connected by interosseous ligaments which, at the -.iiiii' time that they bind the bones together, admit of a certain amount of 1 Kick : (.'oiiipfiiiliiim tier Phygiologie drs Menxchen, "Wien, 1891. •Ill THE ACTION OF LOCOMOTOR MECHANISMS. 415 play, the extent of the movement varying with the character of the surfaces and the length of the ligaments, 4. Union by Joints ( Diarthrosis). — The adjacent surfaces of most of the bones are so formed as to permit of close contact and freedom of movement in special directions. The parts of the bones entering into the joint are clothed with very smooth cartilage, and the joint-surfaces are lubricated by synovial fluid, a viscid liquid secreted by a delicate membrane which lines the fibrous capsule by which the joint is surrounded. The joint-capsule is firmly attached to the bones at the margin of the articular cartilages, and, at the same time that it completely surrounds and isolates the joint-cavity, it helps to bind the bones together. The bones are further united by strong ligaments, in some cases within aud in other cases without the capsule. These ligaments are so placed that they are relaxed in certain positions of the joints and tightened in others ; they guide and limit the movements of the joints. The joint-surfaces always touch, although usually the parts in contact change with the position of the joint. If continuous contact of the joint-surfaces is to be maintained and free movement is to take place in special directions, it is evident that the opposing surfaces must not only be so constructed that they shall fit each other with great accuracy, but also have forms especially adapted to the move- ments peculiar to each of the joints. The different joints exhibit a great variety of movements aud may be clas- sified as follows : gliding joints, hinge joints, condyloid joints, saddle joints, ball-and-socket joints, pivot joints. For a description of the structure and the peculiarities of these joints the student is referred to works on anatomy.1 The contact of the surfaces of the joint is secured in part by the fibrous capsule, in part by the joint ligaments, and in part by the tension of the muscles. The elastic muscles are attached under slight tension, and, moreover, during wak- ing hours are kept slightly contracted by tonus impulses of reflex origin. Another less evident but no less important condition is the atmospheric pres- sure. The capsule fits the joint closely and all the space within not occupied by the bones is filled by cartilages, fibrous bauds, fatty tissues and synovial fluid. The joint is air-tight, and, as was first demonstrated by the Weber brothers, the atmospheric pressure keeps all parts in close apposition. This force is sufficiently great in the case of the hip-joint to support the whole weight of the leg even after all the surrounding soft parts have been cut through. The proof that the air-pressure gives this support is found in the fact that the head of the femur maintains its place in the acetabulum after all the soft parts which surround the joint have been divided, but falls out of its socket if a hole be bored in the acetabulum and air be permitted to enter the cavity of the joint. Though the air-pressure keeps the bones in constant contact it offers no resistance to the movements peculiar to the joints. The movements of the hones arc effected chiefly by muscular contractions, but the direction and extent of the movements arc for the most part deter- mined by the form of the joint-surfaces and the limitations to movement 1 Quoin's Anatomy, vol. ii. pt. 1 ; Grm/s Anatomy; Horrids Anatomy. Ill) AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. which result from the method of attachment of the ligaments. The follow- ing kinds of movement are possible : (a) angular, in which the angle formed by the longitudinal axis of two bones changes, as in flexion and extension or abduction and adduction ; (b) circumduction, in which the longitudinal axis of a bone describes the sides of a cone, the apex of which is in the joint ; (c) rotation, in which ;i bone moves about its longitudinal axis ; (d) gliding, in which a bone so moves as to change its position with reference to its neighbor, without rotation or change of angle. As a matter of fact, most of the movements that are made are the resultant of two or of all of these simple motions. In the gliding joints, in which the articular surfaces are nearly flat (as in the case of the joints between the articular processes of the verte- bra?, and the carpal and tarsal joints), a sliding movement may occur in various directions, and a rotation movement is possible ; but the extent of these movements is very slight, being limited by the strong capsule and ligaments. Singe joints have but a single axis of motion, because the convex and some- what cylindrical surface of one bone fits quite closely the concave surface of the other, and because of tense lateral ligaments which permit of movements in only a single plane. The joint between the humerus and the ulna at the elbow is an example. In this case only flexion and extension are possible, although a slight obliquity of the surfaces causes the head to move in flexion toward the middle line of the body, which is interpreted by some as a screw movement. In this joint the limits of motion arc determined by the contact of the coronoid and olecranon processes of the ulna with the bone in the cor- responding fossa? of the humerus, as well as by the resistance of capsule and ligaments. The knee-joint l is a less simple form of hinge joint. The pres- ence of the semilunar cartilages and the shape of the joint-surfaces cause flexion to be produced by the combined action of sliding, rolling, and rotation movements. In complete extension the lateral ligaments and the posterior and anterior crucial ligaments are put on the stretch, and there is a locking of the joint, no rotation being possible ; in complete flexion, on the other hand, the posterior crucial ligament is tight, but the others are sufficiently loose to allow of a considerable amount of pronation and supination. In the condyloid joint the articulating surfaces are spheroidal, as in the case of the metacarpo- and metatarso-phalangeal joints. These exhibit all forms of angular movement and circumduction. In the saddle-joint there is a double axis of motion — e. g., the articulation of the trapezium with the first meta- carpal bone of the thumb permits of movement aboul an axis extending from before backward, and another, at nearly right angles to this, extending from side to side. All modes of angular movement are possible with such a joint. The ball-and-socket joint, of which the shoulder- and hip-joints are exam- ples, permits of the greatest variety of movements, any diameter of the head 1 W. Braunne and Fischer have studied with mathematical accuracy the construction and movements of many of the joints of the human body. Their articles are published in the Abhancttungen dor math.-phys. Classe der konigl. Siichsischer Geselhchaft der Wissenschaften, Bd. xvii. and others. THE ACTION OF LOCOMOTOR MECHANISMS. 417 of the bone serving as an axis of rotation. The pivot-joint allow- of rotation only; the atlanto-axial and radio-ulnar joint.- may be placed in this class. Method of Action of Muscles upon the Bones. — The bones can be looked upon as levers actuated by the forces which are applied at the points of attachment of the muscles. All three forms of levers are represented in the body; indeed, they may be illustrated in the same joint, as the elbow. An example of a I era- of the first class, in which the fulcrum is between the power and the resistance, is to be found in the extension of the forearm in such an act as driving a nail : the inertia of the hammer, hand, and forearm offers the resistance, the triceps muscle acting upon the olecranon gives the power, and the trochlea, upou which the rotation occurs, is the fulcrum. The balancing of the head upou the atlas is another example: the front part of the head and face is the resistance, the occipito-atlantoid joint the fulcrum, and the muscles of the neck the power. In the case of a lever of the second order, the resistance is between the ful- crum and the power ; for example, when the weight of the body is being raised from the floor by the hands : the fulcrum is where the hand rests on the floor, the weight is applied at the elbow-joiut, aud the power is the pull of the triceps on the olecranon. The raising of the body on the toes is another ex- ample : the fulcrum is at the place where the toes are iu contact with the floor, the resistance is the weight of the body transmitted through the tibia to the astragalus, and the power is applied at the point of attachment of the tendo Achillis to the os calcis.1 The raising of a weight in the hand by flexion of the forearm through contraction of the biceps gives an example of a lever of the third order, in which the power is applied between the fulcrum and the weight. This form of lever, because of the great length of the resistance arm, as compared with the power arm, is favorable to extensive and rapid movements, and is the most usual form of lever in the body. The power is applied to best advantage when it is exerted at right angles to the direction of a lever, as in the case of the muscles of mastication and of the calf of the leg. If the traction be exerted obliquely, the effect is the less the more acute the angle bit ween the tendon of the muscle and the bone; for example, when the arm is extended the flexor muscles work to great disad- vantage, ibr a large part of the force is expended in pulling the ulnar and radius against the humerus, and is Lost Ibr movement, but as the elbow is flexed the force is directed more and more nearly at right angle- t.. the bones of the forearm, and there is a gain in leverage, which is of course again decreased as flexion is completed. This gain in Leverage which accompanies the shortening of the muscles i^ the more important, since the power of the muscle is greatest when the muscle has its normal length, ami continually lessens as the muscle shortens in contraction. There are a number of special arrangements which help to increase the leverage of the muscles by lessening the obliquity of attachment — viz. the enlarged heads of the bones, and in some 1 Certain observers would class this movement as an example of a lever of the first class (Ewald: I'llii'/rr's Archiv, 189. 3 ha Methode graphique, 1885. ' Marey: M&hode graphique (supplement), 1885 ; Muybridge: The Horse in Motion, as Shown by Instantaneous Photography, 1882. VOICE AND SPEECH. 421 ing causes an up-and-down and a lateral sway with each step. Were the legs without joints, like stilts, these oscillations would be very great, especially when the step was long; as a matter of fact, they are slight. The tendency for the centre of gravity to move from side to side as the Legs alternately push the body forward is partly balanced by the swing of the opposite arm ; and the vertical oscillations are minimized by the fact that the leg which is about to receive the weight flexes as the centre of gravity moves forward and comes over it, and extends as it passes on to be received by the other leg. The path taken by the centre of gravity during walking is a complicated one. If referred to the plane in which the body is moving, it describes for one double step an oval ; projected on the horizontal and frontal planes, its path has the form of the sign of infinity, oc. The rate of movement influences its position in special parts of the curve.1 In running, the body is inclined more than in walking, and the legs are more flexed in order that the extension movement of the back leg, which drives the body forward, may be more effective. In running, the body is pro- pelled by a series of spring-like movements and there are times when both feet are off the ground, the back leg leaving the ground before the other touches it. B. Voice and Speech. 1. Structure of the Larynx. Voice-production. — The human voice is produced by vibration of the true vocal cords, normally brought about by an expiratory blast of air passing between them while they are approximated and held in a state of tension by muscular action. Mere vibration of the cords could produce but a feeble sound; the voice owes its intensity both to the energy of the expiratory blast (Helmholtz)2 and to the reinforcement of the vibrations by the resonating cavities above and below the cords. A true conception of the action of the larynx can only be gained by a pre- liminary study of the organ in situ, in its relations with the trachea, pharynx, tongue, extrinsic muscles, and hyoidean apparatus. Removed from its con- nections, the larynx, in vertical transverse section, is seen to be shaped some- what like an hour-glass, the true vocal cords forming the line of constriction half way between the top of the epiglottis and the lower border of the cri- coid cartilage (Fig. 208). In median vertical section the axis of the larynx above the vocal cords extends decidedly backward, and below the cords the axis is nearly perpendicular to the plane in which they lie. The epiglottis is an ovoid lamella of elastic cartilage, shaped like a shoe-horn, that leans backward over the laryngeal orifice so that the observer must look down obliquely in order to inspect the cavity of the larynx (Fig. 212.) The mucous membrane is thickened into a slight prominence, known as the "cushion," at the base of 'Fischer: Abhandl, /< AX TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. the epiglottis. The epiglottis, which is extremely movable in a median plane, mav lie lilted backward so as to close completely the entrance into the larynx. Functions of the Epiglottis. — One function of the epiglottis seems obviously to serve as a cover for the superior entrance of the larynx, over which it is said to shut in the act of swallowing. But it is found that deglutition occurs in a normal manner when the epiglottis is wanting or is too small to cover the aperture, the sphincter muscles surrounding the latter being capable of pro- tecting the larynx against the entrance of foreign substances. It is held by some that the epiglottis has an important influ- ence in modifying the voice according as it more or 1< iss completely covers the exit to the column of vibrating air. It is also held that the epiglottis acts as a sort of sounding- board, taking up and reinforcing the vibra- tions of the air-column impinging against it.1 Sweeping downward and backward from Fig. 208.— vertical transverse section of each edge of the epiglottis is a sheet of the larynx (after Testut) : 1. posterior face of i ,1 • / ,,• r u epiglottis, with r, us cushion; 2, aryteno- mucous membrane, the ary-epigloUic fold, epiglottic fold; 3. ventricular band, or false which forms the lateral rim of the superior vocal cord; 4, true vocal cord; 5, central . . i i • i i of Merkei; 6, ventricle of larynx, with aperture of the larynx and which ends in, 6', its ascending pouch ; 7, anterior portion an(J covers posteriori V, the arytenoid carti- of cricoid; 8, section of cricoid; 1), thyroid, mi i i • l cut surface; 10, thyrohyoid membrane; 11, lages. Ihe rounded prominence on the pos- thyrohyoid muscle; L2, aryteno-epiglottic terior cornei- of this fold is made l)V the car- muscle; 13, thyro-arytenoid muscle, with _ t J 13', its inner division, contained in the vocal tilagc of Saiit< >i*i hi, and a sce< >nd, less marked, cord;n,cnc,,thyroidnn1sc,e K, Md.lottic m „,,,,.„.,] tO it, bv the eartilaqeof portion ol Larynx; l6,cavityoi tin' trachea. '^ » ' & •> Wrisberg (Fig. 215). Looking down into the larynx, it is seen that its lateral walls approach each other by the develop- ment on each side of a permanent ridge of mucous membrane, known as the ventricular band or false vocal cord (Fig. 208). Ventricular Bands and Ventricles of Morg-agrii. — The ventricular bands or false mral cords arise from the thyroid cartilage near the median line, a shorl distance above the origin of the true cords. They are inserted into the arytenoid cartilages somewhat below the apices of the latter. Their free bor- der Is more or less ligamentous in structure. They are brought into contact by the sphincter muscles of the larynx, and thus protect the glottis. It has even been stated that, in paralysis of the true cords, they may be set in vibra- tion and be the seat of voice-formation. So-called "(edema of the glottis" is chiefly due to accumulation of fluid in the wide lymph-spaces found in the false cords. 1 Mills: Journ. of Physiology, 1883, vol. iv. p. 135. VOICE AND SPEECH. 123 Tlie ventricular bands are parallel with and just above the true vocal cords, from which they are separated by a narrow slit. They do not, however, reach so near the middle line as the true cords, which can be seen between and below the bands. The ventricular bands project more or less into the cavity of the larynx like overhanging lips, so that each band forms the inner wall of a space closed by the true vocal cords below, and communicating with the cavity of the larynx through the narrow slit above mentioned. The spaces thus bounded internally by the false cords are known as The Ventricles of Morgagni (Fig. 208). — Xo complete explanation has been offered as to the purposes served by the ventricles of Morgagni and the false vocal cords. Numerous mucous and serous glands seated in the ventricular bands pour their secretions into the ventricles, whence the fluid may be trans- mitted by the overhanging lips of the ventricular bands to the true vocal cords; hence, an important function of the former structure, probably, is to supply to the vocal cords the moisture necessary to their normal action. The secretion contained within the ventricle is protected by the ventricular band from the desiccating influence of the passing air-currents. The existence of the ventricular spaces also permits free upward vibration of the true cords. The ventricles of Morgagni in some of the lower animals, as the higher apes, communicate with extensive cavities which serve an obvious purpose as reso- nating chambers for the voice, and perhaps the preservation of this function in the ventricles themselves is still of importance in the human being. It is nol improbable that the ventricular bands find their most important function as sphincters of the larynx, the superior opening of which may be firmly occluded by their approximation. The well-known fact that during strong muscular effort the breath is held from escaping is, according to Brunton and Cash,1 due to the meeting of the false cords in the middle line. The overhanging shape of the cords allows them to be readily separated by an inspiratory blast, but causes them to be more firmly approximated by an expiratory effort. This mechanism recalls the mode of action of the semilunar valves of the heart. The true vocal cords arise from the angle formed by the sides of the thyroid cartilage where they meet in front, a little below its middle point, and, passing backward, are inserted into the vocal processes of the arytenoid cartilages. The aperture between the vocal cords and between the vocal processes of the arytenoids is known as the glottis or rima glottidis (Figs. 214, 215). Since, as will be seen later, the vocal cords may be brought together while the vocal pro- cesses of the arytenoids are widely separated at their base-, the space between the cords themselves is sometimes called the rima uocalis and thai between the vocal processes the rima respiratoria. In the adult male the vocal cords measure about 15 millimeters in length and the vocal processes measure 8 millimeters in addition. In the female the cords are from 10 to 11 millimeters in length. The freeedges of the cord are thin and straight and are directed upward; their median surfaces are flattened. Each cord is composed of a dense bundle of fibres of yellow elastic tissue, 1 Brunton and (ash : Journ. Anal, and Phx/8., 1883, vol. xvii. 424 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OE PHYSIOLOGY. which fibres, though having a general longitudinal course, are interwoven, and send off shoots laterally into the subjacent tissue. The compact ligament, known commonly as the " vocal cord," forms only the free edge of a reflexion from the side wall of the larynx. This reflexion is wedge-shaped in a vertical, transverse section and contains much clastic tissue and the internal and part of the external thyroarytenoid muscle (Fig. 208). This whole structure properly forms the vocal cord, and by contraction of its contained muscle its thickness and vibrating qualities may be greatly modified. Like the trachea, the larynx, with the exception of the vocal cords, is lined Fig. 209.— Cartilages of the larynx, separated (Stoerk): 1, epiglottis; '_', petiolus; 3, median in itch (if thyroid; 1, superior cornu of thyroid; 5, attachment of stylo-pharyngeus muscle; 6, origin of thyroepiglottic ligament; 7, origin of the thyro-arytenoid muscle; 8, origin of true vocal conl ; 9, interior cornu of thyroid ; 10, car- tilage of Wrisherg : 11, cartilage of Santorini ; 12, 12', arytenoid cartilages, showing attachments of the transverse arytenoid muscle ; 13, 13', pro- . . -u- muscularis, showing attachments of the posterior and lateral cricoarytenoid muscles; II. base Of the arytenoid cartilage; 15, vocal pro- cesses of the arytenoids ; III, articular surface for the base Of the arytenoid cartilage ; 17, posterior view of cricoid cartilage, with outline of attach- iiii ill of the posterior cricoarytenoid muscle; 18, articular surface for inferior cornu of thyroid cartilage. Fig. 210.— Cartilages and ligaments of the larynx, posterior view (after Stoerk): 1, epiglot- tis; 2, cushion of the epiglottis; 3, cartilage of Wrisherg; 4, ary-epiglottic ligament; 5, 8, mucous membrane ; 6, cartilage of Santorini; 7, arytenoid cartilage; i», its processus muscularis; 10, crico- arytenoid ligament; 11, cricoid cartilage; 12, in- ferior cornu of thyroid cartilage; 1"', posterior superior cerato-cricoid ligament; 13', posterior inferior cerato-cricoid ligament; 14, cartilages of the trachea; 1">, membranous portion of trachea. by columnar, ciliated epithelium, the direction of whose movement is upward toward the pharynx. The vocal cords are covered by thin, flat, stratified epi- thelium. The inner surface of the epiglottis, the walls of the ventricles, and the ventricular bands contain much adenoid tissue, the spaces of which are apt to become distended with fluid, giving rise to oedema of those parts. The whole mucous membrane of the larynx, except over the vocal cords, is richly supplied witli glands both mucous and serous in character. VOICE AND SPEECH. 425 Cartilages of the Larynx. — The mechanism of the larynx is supported by a skeleton composed of several pieces of cartilage. The lowermost of these cartilages is the cricoid cartilage, so called from its resemblance to a signet ring (Fig. 209). The cricoid cartilage is situated above the topmost ring of the trachea to which it is attached by a membrane. The vertical measurement of the cricoid cartilage is about one inch on its posterior, and one-quarter inch on its anterior surface. Superior to, and partly overlapping the cricoid, is the thyroid cartilage, which forms an incomplete ring, being deficient posteriorly (Fig. 209). The free corners of the thyroid behind are prolonged upward or downward into projections known as the cornua. The upper pair are attached to the extremities of the greater cornua of the hyoid bone, while by the inner surface of the ends of the lower cornua the thyroid is articulated with the cricoid cartilage and rotates upon it around an axis drawn through the points of articulation. The lower anterior border of the thyroid cartilage is evenly concave, but its upper border has a deep narrow notch in the middle line. The upper half of the thyroid in front projects sharply forward in an elevation known as Adam's apple (pomum Adami), which is much more marked in adult males than in females. The elliptical space between the cricoid and thyroid cartilages in front is covered by a membrane. Adam's apple, the anterior part of the cricoid ring, and the space between the two, can easily be felt iu the liv- ing subject ; they rise perceptibly toward the head with each swallowing movement. The arytenoid cartilages are two in number and are similar in shape (Figs. 209, 210). Each cartilage, which has somewhat the form of a triangular pyramid, is seated on, and articulates with, the highest point on the posterior part of the cricoid cartilage some distance from the middle line. Of the free faces of the pyramid, one looks backward, one toward the middle line, and the third outward and forward. Each face is more or less concave. The apex of each arytenoid cartilage is capped by a small body called the cartilage of San- torini or, from its bent shape, cornicutum laryngis (Figs. 209. 210). Outside and in front of the latter is the minute cuneiform cartilage or cartilage of Wrisberg, enclosed in the ary-epiglottic fold. The lateral posterior corner of the arytenoid cartilage forms a blunt projection which serves for the attach- ment of muscles, the proces.su* muscularis. The anterior, lower, and median part of each cartilage is of especial interest, since it serves for the posterior attachment of the vocal cord ; it is known as the processus rocalis. The thyroid and cricoid cartilages and the body of the arytenoids are of hyaline cartilage, and tend to become ossified in middle life. The other carti- lages and the vocal processes of the arytenoids are composed of the elastic variety. The Muscles of the Larynx may be divided into two classes — the extrinsic and the intrinsic; the former find their origin outside the larynx, and the latter both arise and are inserted within it. Extrinsic Muscles. — To this group belong the %terno-hyoidi the stemo-ihy- roid, and the omo-hi/oid muscles, which depress the larynx or hyoid bone; the thyro-hyoid muscle, which depresses the hyoid bone or elevates the thyroid 426 AN AMERKA.X TEXT-ROOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. cartilage. To the elevators of the larynx belong the genio-hyoid, the mylo- hyoid, the digastric, the stylo-hyoid, and the hyo-glossus. The muscles of the palate and the constrictors of the pharynx enter into coordinated action with the above. When loud Is passing through the pharyx in the act of swallowing, the hyoid bone is drawn upward and forward, raising the larynx with it; the tongue is thrown backward so that the epiglottis covers the entrance into the larynx, and the constrictors of the larynx contract, completely closing the entrance into that organ. Tin- intrinsic muscles of the larynx an; the crico-thyroids, the lateral crico- arytenoids, the postc -lor crieo-arytenoids, the arytenoid, the aryteno-epiglot- tideans, and the tkyro-arytenoids ; all being in pairs except the arytenoid, which crosses the middle line. The crico-thyroid muscle arises from the front and side of the cricoid cartilage and, passing upward and backward, is inserted into the lower edge of the thyroid cartilage ( Fig. 211). The action of the crico- thyroid muscle is to diminish the distance between the thyroid and cricoid car- tilage- in front, either by depressing the front of the thyroid or by elevating that of the cricoid cartilage, or both. In the first case the distance between the anterior attachment of the vocal cords and the vocal processes of the arytenoid cartilages is increased by movement of the thyroid, and in the second case the same effect is produced by backward rotation of the edge of the cricoid upon which the arytenoid cartilages are seated (Fig. 210). The muscle, therefore, is a tensor of the vocal cords. It is, probably, the mechanism we ordinarily use in raising the pitch of the voice when the vocal machinery has been " set " by the other muscles (see below). If the fingers be placed on the cricoid ring and on the /minimi Adami while the ascending scale is sung in the middle chest register, both descent of the fronl of the thyroid and ascent of the cricoid can be made out. The lateral crico-arytenoid muscle arises from the upper, lateral border of the cricoid Fig. 211.— Lateral view of the ., , . , , , , , cartilages of larynx with the crico- cartilage, and passes upward and backward to be thyroid muscle (Quain'* Anatomy, iDserted into the outer edue of the arytenoid car- after Willis): I, crico-thyroid mus- crico-thyroid membrane ; 3, tilage, on and in front of the lateral prominence cricoid cartilage; 4, thyroid carti- /pj gjgy Its main action is to wheel the lage ; 5, upper rings of the trachea. v vocal process of the arytenoid toward the middle line and thus approximate the vocal cords. The posterior crico-arytenoid is a large muscle, which rises from the median posterior surface of the cricoid car- tilage and passes upward and outward to be inserted into the outer surface of the arytenoid cartilage, behind and above the insertion of the lateral crico-arytenoid (Fig. 213). Its action is to turn the vocal processes outward and thus abduct the vocal colds. The posterior crico-arytenoid occupies an important position in the group of respiratory muscles; during vigorous inspiration it is brought into action VOICE AND SPEECH. 427 and widens the glottis. Paralysis of this muscle is a most serious condition, since it is followed by approximation of, and inability to separate, the vocal cords. The arytenoid, or transverse or posterior arytenoid muscle, the single unpaired Fig. 213.— Larynx with its muscles, posterior view (Stoerk) : 1, epiglottis ; 2, cushion ; 3, ary- epiglottic ligament; 4, cartilage of Wrisberg; 5, cartilage of Santorini ; 6, oblique arytenoid muscles; 7, transverse arytenoid muscle; 8, posterior crico-arytenoid muscle; 9, interior cornu of thyroid cartilage; 10, cricoid car- tilage; 11, posterior inferior cerato-ericoid lig- ament; 12, cartilaginous portion; 13, mem- branous portion of trachea. Fig. 212. — Larynx and its lateral muscles after removal of the left plate of the thyroid cartilage (Stoerk) : 1, thyroid cartilage ; 2, thyroepiglottic mus- cle; 3, cartilage of Wrisberg; 4, ary-epiglottic mus- cle; 5, cartilage of Santorini ; 6, oblique arytenoid muscles; 7, thyroarytenoid muscle; 8, transverse arytenoid muscle; 9, processus muscularis of aryte- noid cartilage ; 10, lateral crico-arytenoid muscle ; 11, posterior crico-arytenoid muscle; 12, crico-thyroid membrane; 13, cricoid cartilage; 14, attachment of crico-thyroid muscle; 15, articular surface for the inferior cornu of the thyroid cartilage; 16, crico- tracheal ligament; 17, cartilages of trachea; 18, membrane ms part of trachea. muscle of the larynx, is a considerable band passing across the middle line from the posterior surface of one arytenoid cartilage to that of the other (Fig. 213). Its action is to draw the arytenoid cartilages together in the middle line and approximate the vocal processes; its action is essential in closing the glottis. In the resting larynx the arytenoid cartilages are kept apart by the elastic tension of the parts. The aryteno-epiglottidean, sometimes culled the oblique arytenoid, muscles consist of two bundles of fibres seated upon the surface of the arytenoid muscle (Fig. 213). Each muscle arises from the outer posterior angle of the arytenoid cartilage, and, passing upward and inward, crosses in the middle line partly to be inserted into the outer and upper part of the opposite cartilage, partly to penetrate the ary-epiglottic fold as far as the epiglottis, and the remainder to join some fibres of the thyro-arytenoid muscle. The action of the aryteno-epiglottidean muscles is t<> close the glottis. The tivyro-arytenoid is a muscle of complex mechanism, usually described as formed of two parts, an external and an internal. The external thyro-arytenoid arises from the lower \-2s AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. part of the angle of the thyroid cartilage ; its fibres pass, for the most part, backward and somewhat upward and outward to be inserted into the outer edge of the arytenoid cartilage and its lateral processus musoularis (Figs. 208, 214). Some of its bundles of fibres, however, have different directions, and a portion of them pass upward into the ventricular bands. The internal thyro- arytenoid, wedge-shaped in transverse section, lies between the muscular divis- ion just described and the vocal ligament, by which its thin median edge is covered. The internal thyro-arytenoid arises from the anterior angle of the thvroid cartilage and is inserted into the processus vocalis and the outer face of the arytenoid cartilage. Certain fibre-bundles of this, as of the external division of the muscle, pass in various directions, some of them being inserted into the free border of the vocal cord. The action of the muscle is, on the whole, to draw the arytenoids forward and thus relax the vocal cords; but, by its contraction, the cords may also be approximated and their thickness, and probably their elasticity, extensively modified. Specific Actions of the Laryngeal Muscles. — To sum up the various effects of the muscular action on the larynx : A sphincter action of the larynx is brought about by the combined contraction of all the muscles with the exception of the crico-thyroids and the posterior crico-arytenoids; the vocal cords are adducted and the glottis nar- rowed by the transverse and oblique ary- tenoids, the external thyro-arytenoids, m.thy.ar.i. anc] tne lateral crico-arytenoids ; the m.thy.ar.e. vocal cords are abducted and the glottis m.thy.ar. widened chiefly or wholly by the poste- rior crico-arytenoids ; the vocal cords are made tense by contraction of the crico-thyroids; the vocal cords are slack- ened by the combined action of the sphincter group and especially by the external thyro-arytenoids. It will easily be seen that in the larynx, as in the skeleton at large, the efficiency of any single muscle involves the action of accessory muscles ; thus, contraction of the crico-thyroid could have little effect in tightening the vocal cords were not the arytenoid cartilages fixed !>\ contraction of the posterior crico-arytenoid and arytenoid muscles. Nerve-supply of the Larynx. — The larynx receives its nerve-supply from the superior and the inferior or recurrent laryngeal nerves. The extremely sensitive surface of the mucous membrane of the organ above the vocal cords i- supplied by sensory filaments of the superior laryngeal nerve. The superior laryngeal also supplies motor fibres to the crico-thyroid muscle, whose action as a tightener of the vocal cords is peculiar. All the other muscles of the Pig. 214.— Diagram to illustrate the thyro-aryte- noid muscles ; the figure represents a transverse section of the larynx through the bases of the arytenoid cartilages (redrawn from Foster): Ary, arytenoid cartilage; p.m., processus muscularis; p.v, processus vocalis; Th, thyroid cartilage; c.v, vocal cords; CE is placed in the oesophagus; m.thy.ar.i, internal thyro- arytenoid muscle; m.thy.ar.e, external thyro-arytenoid muscle; m.thiuir.rp, part of the thyro-ary-epiglottic mus- cle, cut more or less transversely; m.ar.t, trans- verse arytenoid muscle. VOICE AND SPEECH. 429 larynx receive their motor impulses from the inferior laryngeal nerve. Much of the nervous mechanism of the larynx is still in dispute. Laryngoscopic Appearance of the Larynx. — Much may be learned by inspection of the larynx during life by means of the laryngoscopic mirror. It is not difficult for an observer to examine his own larynx by placing himself before a second mirror in which may be seen the image reflected from the laryngoscope. To inspect the larynx the tongue must be held well out so as to pull forward the epiglottis, then the structures below appear in the laryngoscopic mirror in reversed position. Beneath the middle of the epiglottis the cushion may be seen as a slight swelling, and continuing downward and backward from the edges of the cartilage, may be seen the ary-epiglottic folds, each marked at its extremity by two rounded nodules, the cartilages of AVris- bergand Santorini (Fig. 215). In quiet breathing the glottis is nearly stationary and opened to the extent of from 3 to 5 millimeters. The vocal cords bounding it look white and glistening in contrast with the red color of the general mucous membrane. The cartilages of Santorini are several millimeters apart, and a sheet of mucous membrane reaches from one to the other. The ventricular 17 Pig. 215. — The laryngoscopic image in easy breathing (Stoerk): 1, base of the tongne; 2, median glosso-epiglottic ligament ; 3, vallecula; -l, lateral glosso-epiglottic ligament; 5, epiglottis; 6, cushion of epiglottis; 7, cornu major of hyoid bone; 8, ventricular band, or false vocal cord; 9, true vocal cord; opening of the ventricle of Morgagni seen between 8 and 9; 10, folds of mucous membrane ; it, sinus pyriformis; 12, cartilage of Wrisberg ; L8, aryteno epiglottic fold; L4, rima glottidis; 15, arytenoid carti- lage ; 16, cartilage of Santorini ; 17, posterior wall of pharynx. bands are seen as red shelves reaching to the outer margin of the shining cords and separated from the latter by a dark line which is the entrance into the ventricles of Morgagni. When a deep inspiration is taken the glottis is widely opened, even to the extent of half an inch; an angle is formed between the vocal process of the arytenoid and the vocal cord, the space between the cartilages of Santorini is widened, and the rings of the trachea, and ex-en its bifurcation may he seen below. With the succeeding expiration the glottis again becomes narrow. When the voice is sounded the picture at once changes. The space between the cartilages of Santorini is obliterated, the vocal processes and cords are 130 AN AMERTCAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. brought together, and the whole rim of the glottis or the vocal cords alone, according to the pitch of the note, may be seen to vibrate. 2. The Voice. The vocal machinery consists of — (1) the motive power or breath ; (2) the larynx, which forms the tone; (3) the chest, the pharynx, the mouth, and the nose, which color the tone; and (4) the organs of articulation.1 The production of voice is undoubtedly accomplished by the vibration of the vocal cords which have previously been approximated in the middle line and made tense through action of the nerve-muscular apparatus already de- scribed. A blast of air from below pressing against the cords so adjusted, causes them to separate and fall into vibration. We have to distinguish in voice the three features of loudness, pitcli, and quality. The loudness of the tone depends on two factors: (1) the strength of the tone-producing blast as determining not only the amplitude of vibration of the vocal cords, but also the energy with which the air is expelled ; (2) the resonance of the two chambers between which the vocal cords are sus- pended, the chest below and the cavities of the head above, whose walls and contained air, by their sympathetic vibration, powerfully reinforce the oscilla- tions imparted to them. The pitch of the voice is determined by the thickness, tension, and length of the vocal cords, conditions which regulate the pitch of the note obtained from any vibrating string. The thickness and the elastic quality of the cords are probably largely under the control of the thyro-arytenoid muscle. The principal tensor of the cords is the crico-thyroid muscle. Other muscles, as described above, may so fix the arytenoid cartilages that their vocal processes may be prevented from taking part in the vibration of the cords throughout the whole and also, possibly, throughout part only of their length. This dampening of the vocal processes of the arytenoids may be accomplished either by pressure applied to them throughout their whole length, in which case the posterior part of the glottis is closed, or they may be pressed together at the tip- alone, leaving the respiratory glottis open as a triangular aperture. Quality. — Variation in the quality of the voice depends on the fact that vibrations of the vocal cords are composite in character, giving rise to notes made up of a fundamental tone combined with upper partial tones (see p. 883). By reason of the varied adjustments that may be imparted to it, the larynx is capable of producing many more qualities of tone than is any artificial instru- ment.2 Change in the size and shape of the resonance-chamber above and below the vocal cords produces a corresponding change in their fundamental notes and, therefore, in the partial tones of the voice which they reinforce by sympathetic vibration (see p. 385). According to Helmholtz,3 the difference in quality between the various vowel sounds of the human voice depends on 1 C. H. Davis: The Voice., 1879. J Helmholtz: Sensations of Time, trans, by Ellis, LS85, p. 98. 3 Op. cit., p. 104. VOICE AND SPEECH. 431 the number and relative prominence of the various overtones determined by- altering the shape and size of the nasal and buccal resonance-chambers. By a simple experiment the production of voice by the vocal cords can easily be illustrated. Take a glass tube, about h inch in diameter and of con- venient length, and press one end firmly against the palmar surfaces of the proximal phalanges of two fingers at their line of division when they are brought together. By blowing smartly into the other end of the tube, a musical note will be produced by the vibration of the folds of the skin be- tween which the air is forced. By relaxing the pressure with which the fingers are held together, the length of the vibrating segment of skin is in- creased and its tension diminished ; its note is accordingly lowered. The reverse conditions are produced when the fingers are held together tightly and the tube applied firmly ; the pitch, of the note is then raised. In these ways the pitch of the note may be varied through two octaves, which is the range of a good singing voice. Various upper partials of the note so produced may be made prominent by sympathetic resonance, if the vibrating air-stream is sent across the opening of a wide-mouthed bottle, of about a pint capacity. The air within the bottle is thrown into sympathetic vibration when its funda- mental tone is contained in the note emitted through the fingers ; when the volume of the air is diminished by slowly pouring water into the bottle, the fundamental tone of the resonator is changed, and it responds to one after another of the partials contained in the musical note. The marvellous adjustment of muscular action by which, at will, notes may be struck of definite pitch and quality, is evidence of an elaborate nervous machinery for the larynx, not only on the efferent side but, possibly through a muscular sense, on the afferent side as well. The various phe- nomena of aphasia, and the anatomical importance of the cerebral areas ■devoted to the elaboration of speech, point in the same direction. The relations between the centres for speech and hearing are most intimate. The ear plays a constant part, as a critical medium, in the tuition of the vocal organs in either speech or song. So-called "dumbness" is the result, usually, not of defects in the vocal organs, but of lack of hearing and, hence, of inability to control by the ear the pitch or quality of the vocal nods. The voice and the larynx of the child foil naturally in a group with those ■of the female as contrasted with the adult male. At the age of puberty a boy's larynx becomes congested and undergoes rapid development. The voice changes rapidly from the juvenile to the adult quality. During this change, the voice frequently "breaks" or rapidly returns from the newly-acquired chest register to the head or falsetto notes of childhood (see p. 133). In hoys who are castrated a good while before the age of puberty is reached, the larynx does not undergo its characteristic development, and the voice remains of n peculiar quality, much valued in some countries in the rendition of vocal music. The practice of castration for eesthetic purposes has, accordingly, in certain districts, long been in vogue. In the female the changes in the larynx and in the voice at puberty are much less marked than in the male. 432 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. Arrangements for Changing the Pitch of the Voice. — As has frequently been mentioned, the vocal cords are stretched, and the pilch of their note is elevated, by contraction of the crico-thyroid muscle. But the change that is thus produced in the tension of the vocal cords is by no means capable of accounting for the full range of pitch which falls within the compass of the voice. When the arytenoid and the crico-arytenoid muscles sufficiently. con- tract, the vocal processes are brought tightly together and their vibration is prevented. Voice-production must then be limited to the vocal cords them- selves, and the stretching action of the crico-thyroids may begin anew and reach its maximum with the glottis so set that only it.- ligamentous borders can vibrate. It can also be seen that the vocal cords themselves may be shortened functionally, or even be broken up into segments, or the main body of the cord be changed in thickness, by contraction of the complex thyro-arytenoid muscles; each such condition would be accompanied by a change in the rate of vibra- tion. We are probably justified in assuming that, when the musical scale is sung, the lowest notes are produced by vibration of the glottic borders through- out their full length, and the elevation of pitch is affected by the gradually- increased tension of the vocal ligaments through the action of the crico-thyroid muscle. This contraction having reached its maximum, the muscle probably relaxes, only to contract again after the vibrating segments of the glottis are shortened by a partial or complete clamping together of the vocal processes in the manner described above. There are thus two or three, or more, adjustments which may be imparted to the vibrating mechanism of the lar- ynx, each of which is distinguished by giving rise to a note of different pitch that may further be altered by action of the crico-thyroid muscle. It might be anticipated that the voice whose pitch was gradually ele- vated in the manner described would suffer some alteration in quality at those points in the scale where there is a change in the set of the lar- vnx producing a shortening of the vibrating segment. Such, indeed, is the fact. Registers. — Long before the invention of the laryngoscope, and before any- thing definite was known of the method of voice-production, it was recognized that in ascending the musical scale there occur certain breaks, as it were, where the voice changes in quality as well as in pitch. It is an object in musical education to render these breaks as little prominent as possible. The kinds of voice included between these breaks were distinguished as the vocal " registers." There is no general agreement among musicians as to how many registers are compassed by the voice, and the nomenclatures used to distinguish them differ in the most confusing fashion. According to some authors, the range of the voice is included within two registers only; more commonly three distinct registers are described, to which, in certain cases, a fourth is said to be prob- able added. The most common designation of the lowesl register is the "chest voice," though it has also been called "thick"1 as distinguished from the " thin " register; another term applied to it is the " long-reed " register as con- 1 Browne and Behnke : Voice, Song, and Speech, 1890, p. 135. VOICE AND SPEECH. 433 trasted with the "short-reed" register.1 The middle register of all voices is by some authors (Garcia,2 Mme. Seiler3) denominated the "falsetto," while other writers use this term to distinguish certain higher notes of the male voice of a peculiar quality not in ordinary use. The third and highest series of vocal sounds is usually known as the " head " register. The lowest or chest register is that used in ordinary life. It is so called from the strong vibrations of the chest-wall which may be felt while the voice is sounded. In passing to the higher register the chest vibration is found to diminish and that of the head bones to increase; in the one case the cavity of the head acts strongly as a resonance chamber, and in the other that of the thorax. According to Madame Seiler, in the lowest register both the vocal ligaments and the vocal processes of the arytenoids vibrate. Iu -the middle register the vocal processes are clamped together and the vibration of the liga- ments seems confined chiefly to their sharp edges; while in the highest register the ligaments themselves appear to be damped throughout the greater part of their length, the vibrations being confined to the edges of an oval slit at their ABC Fig. 216.— The voicing (female) larynx (after Browne and Behnke). A, Small or highest register. B, Upper thin or middle register. C, Lower thin or middle register: T,T, tongue; F,l\ lal.se vocal cords; n.s, cartilages of Santorini ; W, W, cartilages of Wrisberg; V, V, vocal cords. anterior ends (Fig. 216). Within any definite register the quality of individual voices is determined by the size and elasticity of the parts of the larynx, and probably also by peculiarities of the resonating chambers; voices are accord- ingly classified as base, tenor, alto, and soprano. A Whistling Register.— A friend and former pupil of the author's lias the remark- able power of emitting from the larynx nuics which arc indistinguishable in quality from an ordinary whistle. He writes, " The whistle cannot he made to '.slide' into vocal tones of any sort, nor can any other tones he produced simultaneously with it. Its range is about one and a half octaves, or hall' an octave less than my Singing voice. "The lips have nothing to do with the .sound except as their position changes the reso nance-quality of t he tone by ' reinforcement ' or otherwise, for I can whistle almost as read- ily with the teeth closed and the lips wide parted as with the jaws and li|x lirinU closed as in the ordinary position. Any other movement id' the air-column destroys the sound at once." Some years ago the author made a laryngoscopic examination of this larynx while it was in the act id' whistling. No notes were written at the lime, hut the picture reineni bered is that of vocal cords closely approximated, except lor an oval slit between their anterior and middle portions, as in singing head tones, the cords vibrating chiefly along their free edges. Speech. — Language consists, in general, of a combination of short musical sounds, vowel* or sonants, which arc produced purely by vibration of the vocal 1 Mackenzie : Hygiene <) (after Landoisand Stirling) : T, tongue ;p, soft palate; e, epiglottis; g, glot- tis ; h, hyoid bone; 1, thyroid; 2,3, cricoid; 4, arytenoid cartilage. form and size of the mouth-cavity, the position of the tongue, the position of the soft palate separating or allowing communication between the nasal and pharyngeal cavities, undergo a progressive change (Fig. 217). Helmholtz has shown that the vowel sounds owe their differences of quality to the varied resonance of the mouth-cavity, dependent on its shape, through which now one, now another, of the overtones in the note produced by vibration of the vocal cords is reinforced.3 This result is dependent on the fact that when the mouth is set in position for the formation of the various vowel sounds the pitch of its 1 Browne and Behnke : op. ciL, p. 28. 2 Monroe: Manual of Physical and Vocal Training, 1869, p. 51. 3 Helmholtz : loc. cit. VOICE AND SPEECH. 435 fundamental note, or the rate of vibration to which it sympathetically responds, varies accordingly.1 That the resonance of the mouth cavity changes with its shape is illustrated in the various pitch of the notes produced by Hipping the edge of an incisor tooth, the cheek, or Adam's apple with the finger-nail, while the mouth assumes the positions for production of the different vowels. Vowels whose normal pitch is low, as o, u, cannot be sounded easily in the higher part of the musical scale; conversely, high-pitched vowels, as e in feet, lose their character in the lower part of the scale. Language is, therefore, much less distinct in song than in speech.2 It has already been stated that the difference in quality of musical notes depends upon the number and relative intensity of their partial tones, each of which is separated from the fundamental tone by a fixed interval. Since the mouth parts have a fairly fixed position for each vowel sound, the buccal cavity reinforces by sympathetic resonance tones of definite vibration rates. When a given vowel is sounded in different parts of the musical scale, now one, now another partial tone is reinforced, according as its pitch harmonizes with the prime tone of the mouth cavity, so that the interval between the resonated partials and their fundamental tone may change, with corre- sponding change in the quality of the vowel sound. That is, the resonated partial depends not only on its relation to the fundamental, but also on its vibration rate.3 This feature of vocal resonance distinguishes the human larynx from most musical instruments. That the ground is not covered by these facts was shown by Auerbach,4 who demonstrated that the strength of upper partials in vowel sounds depends also on the strength of their production by the vocal cords and, therefore, upon their relation to the fundamental tone. That is to say, the quality of a vowel is dependent not only on the absolute vibration numbers of its upper partials, according to which they are or are not reinforced by the position of the mouth, but also on the relative position of these upper partials as compared with the fundamental tone. The peculiar aesthetic value of the human voice is dependent on the fact that, on account of its varied powers of adjustment, the larynx is capable of pro- ducing many more kinds of tone-quality than any artificial instrument. Helm- holtz5 found no less than sixteen overtones to accompany the fundamental. The posture of the mouth-parts differs markedly when set for the various principal vowel sounds ; but as we know that each vowel sound has several modifications or gradations so that a tone may pass by an easy glide from one to another, so the form of the mouth passes by insensible steps from one vowel position to another. It will be seen later that several articulate sounds play the part now of vowels, now of consonants, according to their position in the syllable or mode of formation. There has also been shown reason lor believ- ing that the form of the chest cavity and the tension of its walls arc factors in determining the pitch of its fundamental tone; so that through the varied 1 Helmholtz: op. cit., p. 108. 'l Op. cil., p. 114. 3 Op. cit., p. 118. 4 Quoted by Griitzner: op. cit., p. 179. 5 Op. cil., p. 103. 436 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. sympathetic resonance of the thorax the reinforcement of laryngeal tones may here be altered somewhat, as in the month itself.1,2 Whispering- is a mode of speech in which noise largely replaces pendular musical vibrations. The glottis remains more or less widely open and the vocal cords are not tense; the vibrations arc produced both in the larynx and in the buccal-pharyngeal chambers. Vowel sounds may be produced in whis- pering as well as in true voice because, from the multitude of irregular vibra- tions, those waves are reinforced which make up the vowel sounds determined by the set of the mouth. Gentle whispering requires much less effort than does speaking, and inspiratory whispering is less easily distinguished from expiratory than is the strained voice of inspiration from the natural sound of expiration. Consonants, as already indicated, may sometimes play the part of vowels, but pure consonants do not appear in syllables except in combinations with vowels, which combinations always carry the syllable accent. Consonants. — The distinction between consonants and vowels lies in the fact that the tones of the latter are produced by vibration of the vocal cords, the parts above which act only as resonance-boxes and modify the sound, and never offer marked obstruction to the exit of air; whereas in the formation of consonants there is some adjustment in the mouth-passage either in the nature of a local narrowing, by which a peculiar noise is added to the vocal sound, or in the nature of a sudden closing or opening of the air-channel by which a characteristic noise is likewise added to the vocal sound. In other words, the parts above the larynx make the sounds of consonants but only modify those of vowels.3 No sharp line of separation can be drawn between vowels and consonants, since certain characters, according to their associations, now fall into one, now into another class. In the classification of consonantal sounds much confusion exists, dependent chiefly on the fact that several letter charac- ters change their modes of formation and expression with their place in the syllable. The same facts, also, are expressed by different authors by different nomenclatures, and sounds occur in one language that are not found in another. Adopting the general classification of (Jriitzner,4 we may divide consonants into the following three groups: 1. Semi-vowels or liquids, which can be used either as vowels or consonants; this group includes the sounds m, n, ng, I, and /•. In expressing the function of a consonant, the letter is not to be sounded as if it stood alone, but its cha- racter given as actually expressed in a syllable; thus the sound of p is not pee, Inn is the abbreviated labial expression, as in pack or jticrv when all the letters are eliminated after the first. Of the liquids the v. m, and ng (sometimes called "resonants") have the nature of vowels when final (as in him, hen, being), and are then produced by vibration of the vocal cords, the lips having previously been closed for the m, and the tongue applied to the roof of the mouth to cut off the exit of air for n and ng ; the expelled air escapes alto- gether through the nose, which acts as a resonance-chamber. Used as conso- 1 Op. cit., p. 93. -.wall and Pollard: Journal of Physiology, 1890. vol. xi., p. 159. "Griitzner: op. cit., \>. 196. 4 Op. cit., p. 197. VOICE AND SPEECH. 437 nants, as in make and no, m and n are seen to have the characters of the second group, — Explosives. L is pronounced somewhat like n, but air is allowed to escape through the mouth on each side of the tongue ; it may be produced either with voice or without voice (in whispers). It may have vowel charac- ters as in play. 11 is characterized as a vibrative and may have several seats of articulation, as by the thrill of the tip of the tongue against the hard palate, or that of the hind part of the tongue against the soft palate, or even by the coarse vibration of the vocal cords themselves. In the first two cases it may be sounded either with or without voice. Its vowel nature is shown in such words as prat/. 2. Explosives, which are produced either when an obstruction is suddenly offered to or removed from the exit of air from the mouth; at the same time a characteristic noise is produced. They may be subdivided according to the place of articulation into labials (p, v) ; linguo-palatals (t, d) ; gutturals (k, g). The similarity in the method of formation of p and b, t and d, k and g, is striking. They are frequently characterized as being formed with or without voice; that is, 6, d, and g require voice for their distinct recognition, and when whispered they are easily mistaken for p, t, k, which latter do not require voire (vibration of the vocal cords) for their recognition. A consonant, then, is said to be formed with voice when it can be rendered distinctly only by an accom- panying vibration of the vocal cords, without voice when articulated clearly without laryngeal aid. The former are sometimes called sonants, the latter surds. This classification only approximates the truth, for the suddenness and energy with which the obstruction to the breath is removed determines our recognition of the consonant irrespective of voice.1 Table of Consonantal Elements? Oral. Place of Articulation. Momentary. Continuous. Surds Sonants Surds sonants (without voice), (with voice), (without voice), (with voice), Nasal. Continuous. Sonants iwitii \ oice] Lips Lips and teeth Tongue and teeth . . . Tongue and hard palate ( forward ) Tongue and hard palate (hack) Tongue, hard palate, and soft palate Tongue and soft palate . Various places t ch f th(in) sh <1| 7 z. r zh, r y. 1 3. Friction sounds or frictionals, often called aspirates, are all noises pro- duced by the expired blast passing through a constriction in its passage, at which point a vibration is set up. No obstruction being offered t" tin' sound, they are known as continuous as distinguished from the momentary sounds of 1 Griitzner, op. cit., pp. 211, 213. * Webster's International Dictionary, 1891, p. lxvi. 438 AN AMERICAN TEXT- BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. group 2. They may be divided into labio-dental JricfionaU, f (without voice) ; r, w (with voice) ; the lingual friciionah 8, fh (as in them); sh, ch soft (with- out voice); ~, / (with voice). The sound of h may be regarded as due to the vibration of the separated vocal cords. It is peculiar, however, in appearing to be formed in any part of the vocal chamber* when it is formed the mouth parts take on no peculiar position, but assume that of the vowel following the //, as hark, hear, etc. V. REPRODUCTION. The principles and problems of Physiology that have been already pre- sented in this work, comprising nutrition and the functions of the muscular and the nervous systems, have reference to the individual man or woman. Through the normal activity of those functions and their appropriate co- ordination the individual lives his daily life and performs his daily tasks as an independent organism. But man is something more than an independent organism ; he is an integral part of a race, and as such he has the instincts of racial continuance. The continuance of the race is assured only by the pro- duction of new individuals, and the strength of the human reproductive instinct is indicated in some measure by the large proportion of energy that is expended by woman in the bearing of children and by both sexes in the nur- ture and education of the young. The function of reproduction is not limited to the daily life and well-being of independent organisms. It has a deeper significance than that. Its essence lies in the fact that it has reference to the species or race. Many of its problems are, therefore, broad ones; they in- clude not only the immediate details of individual reproduction, but larger ones relative to the nature and significance of reproduction and of sex, and to heredity. In the following discussion, while attention will be given chiefly to the facts of individual reproduction, some of the broader applications of the facts will be indicated. A. Reproduction in General. In all forms of organic reproduction the essential act is the separation from the body of an individual, called the parent, of a portion of his own material living substance, which under suitable conditions is able to grow into an inde- pendent adult organism. Among living beings two methods of reproduction are recognized, the asexual and the sexual methods. Both are widespread among animals and plants, but the asexual method is the more primitive of the two and is rela- tively more frequent in low organisms. The sexual method, the only one present in the production of new individuals among the higher animals, has evidently been acquired gradually, and has probably been developed from the asexual method. Asexual Reproduction. — Asexual reproduction, or agamogenesis, i> the chief method of reproduction among unicellular plants and animals, and throughout the plants and in the lower multicellular animals it is important. Among various species it takes various forms, known as fission or division, gemmation or budding, endogenous cell-formation or spore-formation or multi- 439 440 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. pie fission ; but all the varieties are modifications of the simplest form, fission or division. In fission, found only in unicellular organisms and typified in Amoeba, the protoplasm of the single cell, together with the nucleus, becomes divided into two approximately equal portions which separate from one another. In the process no material is lost, and two independent nucleated organisms result, each approximately half the size of the original. The parent has become bodily transformed into the two offspring, which have only to increase in size by the usual processes of assimilation in order themselves to become parents. In higher organisms, even where sexual processes alone prevail in the production of new individuals, the asexual method has per- sisted in the multiplication of the individual cells that constitute the body; embryonic growth is an asexual reproductive process, a continued fission, dif- fering from the amoeboid type in the facts that the resulting cells do not sepa- rate from one another to form independent organisms, but remain closely associated, undergo morphological differentiation and physiological specializa- tion, and together constitute the individual. Likewise in the adult the pro- duction of blood-corpuscles and of epidermis, the regrowth of lost tissues, and the healing of wounds are examples of asexual cell-reproduction. From the standpoint of multicellular growth Spencer and Haeckel have happily termed the process of asexual reproduction in unicellular organisms " discontinuous growth." Sexual Reproduction. — Sexual reproduction, or gamogenesis, occurs in unicellular organisms, where it is known as conjugation, and it is the prevail- ing form of reproduction in most of the multicellular forms. In most of the invertebrate and vertebrate animals it is the sole form of reproduction of individuals. In its simple form of conjugation, typified in the minute monad, Heteromita, it consists of a complete fusion of the bodies of two similar indi- viduals, protoplasm and nuclei, followed by a division of the mass into numerous spore-like particles, each of which grows into an adult Heteromita. In the higher infusorian, Paramaeeium, the fusion of the two similar individ- uals is a partial and temporary one, during which a partial exchange of nuclear material takes place • tin- is followed by separation, after which each individual proceeds to live its ordinary life and occasionally to multiply by simple fission. In the highly specialized sexual reproduction of higher animals, including man. the individuals of the species are of two kinds or sexes, the male and the female, with profound morphological and physiological differences between them ; in each the protoplasm of the body consists of two kinds of cells, somatic cells and germ-cells, the former subserving the nutritive, muscular, and nervous function- of daily life, the latter subserving reproduction. The germ-cells of the male, called spermatozoa, are relatively small and active, those of the female, called ova, are relatively large and passive; the reproductive process consists of a fusi >f a male and a female germ-cell, the essential part being a fusion of their nuclei ; and this is followed by continued asexual cell-division and growth into a new individual. Among both plants and animals it is not B EPR OB UCTJON. 44 1 difficult to find a series of forms showing progressively greater and greater deviations from the typical asexual toward the typical sexual method of reproduction, and the existence of such a series is indicative of the derivation of the latter from the former type. Origin of Sex, and Theory of Reproduction. — It is obvious that the production of new individuals is necessary to the continued existence of any species. It would be interesting to know the origin and significance of the two existing methods of reproduction. Apropos of the asexual process, Leuckart, and especially Herbert Spencer, have pointed out that during the growth of a cell the mass increases as the cube, but the surface only as the square, of the diameter — i. e. the quantity of protoplasm increases much more rapidly than the absorptive surface. It follows from this that during the growth of a unicellular organism a size will ultimately be reached beyond which the cell will not be able to absorb sufficient food for the maintenance of the proto- plasm. In order that growth may continue beyond this point, a division of the cell, which ensures a relative increase of surface over mass, is absolutely necessary. Fission is, therefore, a necessary corollary of growth, and, although we are ignorant of the details of its mechanism, it is conceivable that the method of asexual reproduction arose through causes connected with growth. The explanation of sexual reproduction is much more difficult, for here, in addition to the budding off of the germ-cells from the parental bodies, which has probably the same fundamental cause as fission in unicellular forms, we must account for the differentiation into sexes, the existence of special sexual cells, and the fusion of the male and the female germinal substance; in short, we must account for the conception of sexuality itself and all that it implies. Regarding the origin of sexuality itself, as to the question whether sexuality is an original and fundamental attribute of protoplasm or has been acquired, we may say at once that at present we know really nothing. Yet, whatever view is held as to the origin of sexuality, it seems entirely probable that the method of reproduction known as sexual is a derivative of the method known as asexual — the latter is primitive, the former has arisen from it. From the wide distribution and prominence of the former among vital phenomena we must believe, with biologists generally, that sexual differentiation and sexual processes have arisen from natural causes, and for the reason that sexual repro- duction is of advantage to living beings and to species. In what way it IS of advantage, however, is disputed. Three views, all of which have evidence in their favor and which are not mutually exclusive, are at present engaging the attention of scientific men. The first to be mentioned is the theory advocated by Hensen, Edouard van Beneden, and Butschli, according to whom the fusion of the cells in sexual reproduction exists for the purpose of rejuvenating the living substance. The power possessed by cells of dividing asexually is limited; in time the protoplasm grows old and degenerates; its vital powers an1 weakened, and without help the extinction of the race must follow. But the mingling of another strain with such senescent protoplasm gives it renewed youth and vigor, restores the power of fission, and giants a new lease of life to 442 AN AMERICAN TENT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. the species. From his observations upon the [nfusoria, Maupas1 has brought forward valuable evidence which has been quoted in favor of this view. Sty- lonychia normally produces by fission 130 to 180 generations or individuals, Onyehudromus 140 to 230, and Leucophrys patida 300 to 450, after which con- jugation is necessary to continued division. If conjugation be prevented, the individuals become small, their physiological powers become weakened, their nuclei atrophy, and the chromatin disappears ; all of which changes are evidence of* the (incoming of senile degeneration, and this ultimately results in death. Analogous to this is doubtless the fact, pointed out by Hertwig,2 that in sexual animals an unfertilized ovum within the oviduct soon becomes over-mature and enfeebled, and subsequent fertilization, even though possible, is abnormal. Even if the idea of " rejuvenescence" be regarded as fanciful and as a com- parison rather than an explanation, it seems to be a principle of nature that occasional fusion of one line of descent with another is necessary to continued reproduction and continued life. A second theory, defended by Hatschek and Hertwig, argues that sexual reproduction prevents variation, and thus preserves the uniformity of the race. The mingling of two different individuals possessing different qualities must give rise to an individual intermediate between the parents, but differing from them. Such differences between parents and offspring are numerous, but in a single generation are minute, and they are easily obliterated by a subsequent union, which latter in turn gives rise to other minute differences. Hence sexual reproduction, although constantly producing variations, as constantly eradicates them. and. by striving always toward the mean between two extremes, tends toward homogeneity of the species. The essential truth of such a view seems obvious. A third theory, advocated by Weismann and Brooks, is quite the opposite of the last, and maintains that the meaning of sexual reproduction lies in the production of variations. "The process furnishes an inexhaustible supply of fresh combinations of individual variations." These minute variations, seized upon by natural selection, are augmented and made serviceable, and a variety, better able to cope with the conditions of existence, results. The transformation, not the homogeneity, of the species is thereby assured. The two latter views are not necessarily mutually exclusive. Both claim that fertilization brings into evidence variations. It is quite conceivable that subsequenl fertilizations may obliterate some and augment others, the result of union being the algebraic sum of the characteristics contributed by the two sexes. Primary and Secondary Characters. — In the human species, as in all the higher sexual animals, the characters of sex, anatomical, physiological, and psychological, are divisible into two classes, called primary and secondary. Primary sexual characters arc those that pertain to the sexual organs them- selves and to their functions. They are naturally the most pronounced of all 1 E. Maupas: Archives de Zoologie exp&rirnentale et genSrale, 2e se>ie, vii , 1889. 2 O. uiid II. Hertwig: Experimentelle Studien am thieri&chen Ei for, wahrend uml nnch der Befruchtung, i., 1890. REPR OD UCTION. 443 sexual attributes. Secondary sexual characters comprise those attributes that are- not directly connected with the sexual organs, but that, nevertheless, con- stitute marked differences between the sexes ; such are the greater size and strength of man's body as compared with woman's, the superior grace and delicacy of woman's movements, the deeper, rougher voice of man, and the higher, softer voice of woman. In reality, all secondary sexual characters are accessory to the primary ones, and the greater portion of the present article will be devoted to a discussion of the latter. The primary sexual characters of the male centre in the production of spermatozoa and the process of impreg- nation, those of the female in the production of ova and the care of the devel- oping embryo. Sexual Organs. — Sexual organs are classified into essential and accessory organs. The essential organs are the two testes of the male and the two ovaries of the female. The accessory organs of the male comprise the vasa deferentia, the seminal vesicles, the urethra, the penis, the prostate gland, Cow- per's glands, and the scrotum and its attached parts. The accessory organs of the female comprise the oviducts or Fallopian tubes, the uterus, the vagina, the various external parts included in the vulva, and the mammary glands. During the greater part of life the sexual organs perform but a portion of their duties ; only at intervals, and in some individuals never, do they complete the cycle of their functions by engaging in the reproductive process itself. In the fol- lowing account we shall discuss first the habitual physiology of the organs of the male and of the female, and later their special activities in the repro- ductive process. B. The Male Reproductive Organs. The male reproductive organs, already mentioned, have as their specific functions the production of the essential male germ-cells, the spermatozoa, the production of a fluid medium in which the spermatozoa can live and undergo transportation, the temporary storing of this seminal fluid, and its ultimate transference to the outside world or to the reproductive passages of the female. The Spermatozoon. — Spermatozoa were first discovered by Hainm, a student at Ley den, in 1677. Ilamm's teacher, Leeuwenhoek, 6rs1 studied them carefully. They were long believed to be parasites, even until near the middle of the present century, when their origin and fertilizing function were established. Spermatozoa are cells modified for locomotion and entrance into the ovum. Human spermatozoa are slender, delicate cells, averaging 0.055 millimeter (^j-g- of an inch) in thickness, and consisting of a head, a middle- piece, and a tail (Fig. 218). The head (h) is flattened, egg-shaped, with a thin anterior al'xc and often slightly depressed sides. It terminates anteriorly in a slender, projecting, and sharply pointed thread <»r spear. It consists of a nucleus composed of a dense mass of chromatin and covered l>v an excessively thin layer of cytoplasm, von Bardeleben ' claims the number of chromo- somes in the chromatin after maturation to be eight. 1 K. v. Bardeleben: Verhawtttungen der anatomischen Oeselhchaft; Analomischer An 1892, vii. 444 AN AMERICAN TEXT- BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. The middle-piece (m) is a short, cytoplasmic rod, probably containing a cen- trosome. The tail (/) is a delicate filiform, apparently cytoplasmic structure. and analogous to a single ciliuni of a ciliated cell. The tail is tipped by an excessively fine, short filament, the end-piect (e). The most . abundant of the .-olid chemical constituents of the spermato- zoon is nuclein. probably in the form of nucleic acid, which is found in the head. Other constituents are proteids, prota- mine, lecithin, cholesterin, and fat. The structure and power of movement of the spermatozoon plainly show it to be adapted to activity. It is not burdened by the presence of food-substance within its protoplasm. It is the active clement in fertilization ; it seeks the ovum, and it i- modified from the form of the typical cell for the special purpose of fertilization. The nucleus is the fertilizing agent. The head is plainly fitted for facilitating entrance into the ovum. The tail is a locomotor organ capable of spontaneous movements, and, after expulsion of the semen, it propels the cell, head forward, through the liquid in which it lies. The movement is a complex one, and is effected by the lashing of the tail from side to side, accompanied by a rotary move- ment about the longitudinal axis. The rate of movement has fig. 218.— HamaD been variously estimated at from 1.2 to 3.6 millimeters in the spermatozoa (after minute. Spermatozoa taken directly from the testis are Retziust : .1, seeD en . •' /ace; h, head!; m, quiescent ; normally they begin to move when mixed with renSpTece65 jT^ the secre1 ions of the access017 sexual organs.1 Toward heat, seen from the si.ie. cold, and chemical agents spermatozoa behave like ciliated cell-. Ripe spermatozoa appear to be capable of living for months within the male genital passages, where they are probably quiescent. Outside of the body they have been kept alive and in motion for forty-eight hours. Tt is not certain how long they may remain alive within the genital passages of the human female. Diihrssen- claims to have found motile spermatozoa in the oviduct at leasl three and one half weeks after coition. It seems not improb- able that within the female organs their environment is favorable to a some- what prolonged existence. In this connection it is of interesl to know that spermatozoa capable of fertilizing have been known to live within the recep- taculum ■•« minis of a queen bee for three years. Spermatozoa are produced in large numbers. Upon the basis of observa- tions in several individuals. Lode 3 computes the average production per week as 226,257,000, and in the period of thirty years from twenty-five to fifty- five years of age the total production as 339,385,500,000. This excessive production is an adaptation by nature thai serves a a compensation for the lCf. Walker: Archivfur AnatomU und Physiologic, Anatomischer Abtheilung, 1899, S. 313. 2 Diihrssen: CmtraJblatt fur Gynakologie, 1893, xvii. S. 592. 'A. Lode: Pjiiiger's Archivfur die gesammte Physvdogie, 1891, 1. BEPR OD UCTION. 445 small size of the cells and the small chance, of every cell finding an ovum. Without large numbers fertilization would not be ensured and the continu- ance of the species would be endangered. Maturation of the Spermatozoon. — Considerable theoretical interest attaches to the question of the real morphological value of the spermatozoon. It is undoubtedly a cell, and has arisen by division from one of the testicular cells, called the spermatocyte or sometimes the mother-cell of the spermato- zoon. But is it the morphological equivalent of one of the mother-cells? In most animals, and probably also in man, each spermatocyte gives rise to four spermatids, which grow directly into four spermatozoa. The process of derivation of the spermatozoa may be called, by analogy with the process in the ripening of the ovum, maturation. The details and essence of the process have been much discussed. Van Beneden found in an interesting worm, Asearis, that the number of chromosomes in the nucleus of a single sperma- tozoon is only half that in the original testicular cell ; that is, the process of maturation of the spermatozoon consists in a reduction of the chromosomes by one-half. This discovery has since been extended to many other forms, including mammals and man,1 and it has been shown further that the mature spermatozoon contains only one-half of the number of chromosomes charac- teristic of the tissue-cells of the species in question. In the light of the sub- sequent process of fertilization these facts are interesting. Following Hert- wig and Strasburger, who regard the chromatic substance of the nucleus as the bearer of the hereditary qualities, many biologists now interpret this halving of the chromatin as a provision for the reduction of the hered- itary mass, which later will be restored to its full amount by union with the egg. As we shall see, the maturation of the ovum follows a some- what similar course, and. since the process has been more fully studied there, we shall reserve further discussion until that subject is reached (p. 451). Semen. — Semen consists of spermatozoa, together with liquid and dissolved solids, coming partly from the testes themselves, but secreted chiefly by the accessory sexual glands — namely, the glands within the vasa deferentia, the seminal vesicles, the prostate gland, and Cowper's glands. It is a whitish, viscid, alkaline fluid, with a slight characteristic odor. The amount passed out at any one time has been estimated at between 0.5 and (5 cubic centimeters. Its chemical composition has not been examined exhaustively, lie-ides water, it contains approximately 18 per cent, of solid substances, which comprise nuclein, protamine, proteids, xanthin, lecithin, cholesterin, and other extractives. Cat, and Sodium and potassium chlorides, sulphates, and phosphates. I Jnder proper treat- ment colorless crystals, called Bottcher's crystals, may be obtained from semen. They appear to be a phosphate of a nitrogenous base, which has been called s/),rm- ine. Interest in the semen centres in its histological rather than its chemical features. The fluid portion serves as a vehicle for the transportation of and pos- sibly also for the nutrition of the ripe spermatozoa. Colorless particles, called 1 v. Bardeleben : loc eit. 44G AN AMERICAN TEXT- HOOK OF 1'JI YSIOLOGY. seminal granules, exist in Bemen. They are possibly parts of nuclei of disin- tegrated cells. < Jomparatively little is known of the composition or the specific function of the individual secretions contributed by the various organs. The disintegration of the nutritive cells of the testis probably furnishes some of the nutritive substance of the liquid. Prostatic secretion is viscid, opalescent, and usually alkaline, and contains L.5 per cent, of solids, comprising mainly pro- teids and salts. It contributes at Least a portion of the substance of Bottcher's crystals to the semen, and their partial decomposition is said to be responsible for the characteristic odor of the seminal fluid. The secretion from the seminal vesicles is fairly abundant, is albuminous, and in some animals at least, Buch as the rodents, seems to contain fibrinogen. This enables the fluid to clol after it- reception in the female passages, and thus to prevent loss of spermatozoa. Camus and Gley1 find that this coagulation is caused by a specific fermenl present in the prostatic fluid. Cowper's glands secrete a mucous fluid. By careful experiments upon white rats Steinaeh 2 has shown thai removal of the seminal vesicles and the prostate gland, while not dimin- ishing sexual pa— ion and the ability to perform the sexual act, including the actual discharge of spermatozoa, prevents entirely the fertilization of the ova ; removal of the seminal vesicles alone markedly weakens the fertilizing power of the -emeu, ruder normal circumstances the secretions of these accessory glands arc essential to the motility of the spermatozoa,3 and they may have other important functions. Ivanoh",' however, has been able to impregnate dogs, rabbits, and guinea-pigs artificially by injecting into the vagina spermatozoa taken directly from the epididymis and mixed with a 0.5 per cent, solution of .-odium carbonate. The Testis. — The testes ( Fig. 219, /) are compound tubular glands with a unique structure. Formed early in embryonic life as solid structures, with the seminiferous tubules (to) represented by solid cords of cells, they remain in the embryonic condition until the time of puberty. Some of the cells, the mother-cells of the -pern 1a t ozoa, t hen begin actively to divide, and the result of division with differentiation is the mature spermatozoa. These latter accumulate at the centre of the tubules, the walls being formed largely of the dividing cells or immature spermatozoa. Other cells do not produce spermatozoa, but seem to disintegrate and give rise to the nutritive fluid and nuclear particles that are found mixed with the sperm-cells. From the time of puberty on, usually throughout life, this cellular activity proceeds, the rate and regularity probably varying greatly with individuals and depend- ing largely on the frequency of discharge of the semen. Spermatozoa may be wanting in old men, but they have been found in individuals at eighty or ninety year- of age. The spermatozoa accumulate within the seminal L Camas and Gley: Oomptes rendus de la SocieHS de Biologic, 1896, p. 7*7, and 1897, p. 787. I. Steinaeh: Pfluger'a Archie fiir die gesammte Physiologic, 1894, lvi. ( f. also Rehfisch : V medicinischt Woch L896, xxii. S. 245; and Lode: Sitsungsber. f these, the two corpora cavernosa lie at the sides, and meet each other in the middle line along the upper side of the penis ; the corpus spongi- osum lies in the middle line below, and is pierced throughout its length by the urethra. At its proximal end each corpus is enlarged into a bulbous part, REPR OD UCTION. 449 and is covered by a layer of muscular fibres constituting a distinct muscle — the bulbs of the corpora cavernosa by the ischio-cavernosi (erectores penis), that of the corpus spongiosum (called bulbus urethne\ by the bulbo-cavcrnosu.s (accel- erator urinai). At its distal end each corpus cavernosum terminates bluntly, while the corpus spongiosum projects farther and enlarges to form the extrem- ity of the organ, the glans penis. Each corpus is spongy in consistence, being formed of a trabecular framework of white and elastic connective tissue and plain muscular fibres, with cavernous venous spaces, and it is covered by a tough fibrous tunic. When the spaces are distended with blood the whole organ becomes hard, rigid, and erect in position. The mechanism of erection will be studied more in detail later (p. 463). The penis, especially toward its termination, is beset with end-bulbs, Pacinian bodies, and other nerve-ter- minations, which make it particularly sensitive to external stimulation. C. The Female Reproductive Organs. The female reproductive organs, already mentioned, have as their specific- functions the production of the essential female germ-cells, the ova, and their transference to the uterus, and, if unfertilized, to the outside world ; if the ova are fertilized, other specific functions are the protection and nutrition of the developing embryo, its ultimate transference to the outside world, and the nutrition of the child during early infancy. The Ovum. — The human ovum was discovered in 1827 by von Baer, and it was he who first completely traced the connection between ova in the gene- rative passages and ova in the Graafian follicles of the ovary. The conception of ova as the essential female element had, however, long been held, and Har- vey's dictum of the seventeenth century, / that everything living is derived from an egg (omne vivum ex ovo), is well known. The human ovum, as it comes from the ovary, is a spherical, proto- plasmic cell (Fig. 220), averaging with the zona radiata, approximately 0.2 milli- meter (jyt inch) in diameter. As in other cells, the cell-body may be distin- guished from the nucleus, the proto- Fig. 220,-HTuaan ovum (modified from Na- o ' tr gel): », nucleus (germinal vesicle) containing plasm of the former being called cyto- the amoeboid nucleolus (germinal spot) ; d,deu- 7 t •, n , ,i , toplasmic zone; />. protoplasmic zone; c, zona plasm. In its finer structure the cyto- riuiiatu; v I,(,,vit(.lliu,sl,a(.(, plasm consists of an excessively delicate network of protoplasmic substance. As in other mammalian eggs, it proba- bly contains, adjoining the nucleus, a minute, specially differentiated portion, consisting of a single or double centrosome surrounded by an attraction sphere (Fig. 221, .4). For some distance inward from the border the cytoplasm is pure and transparent, and this portion is often called the protoplasmic zone Vol. IT.— 29 150 AiX AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. (Fig. 220, p). Throughout the centre of the cell, however, it is obscured by the presence of an abundance of yolk-substance, or deutoplasm, from which the corresponding part of the ovum is sometimes called the deutoplasmic /one ((/). Deutoplasm is non-living substance; it consists of granules of yolk imbedded in the meshes of the cytoplasmic network, and, like its ana- logue, the yolk of the hen's egg, it serves as food for the future cells of the embryo. A comparison of the respective amounts of food in the human and the fowl's egg, with the manner of embrvonic development, is suggestive. The chick develops outside the body of the hen, and, therefore, requires a large supply of nutriment, which it finds in the yolk and the white of the egg. The child develops within the mother's body and receives its nourishment from the maternal blood; hence the supply of food within the egg is only enough to ensure the beginning of growth, special blood-vessels being formed to facilitate its continuance. The nudevs (/<), frequently called by its early name, the germinal vesicle, is spherical, and usually occupies a slightly eccentric position. Its protoplasm consists of a network composed of two kinds of material : the more delicate, slightly staining threads are the achromatic substance, the coarser, deeply staining portion, the chromatic substance or chromatin. The former is con- tinuous with, and probably of exactly the same nature as, the cytoplasm. The chromatin is peculiar to the nucleus, and at certain stages in the nuclear history is resolved into distinct granules or filaments, the chromosomes (Fig. 221, A), the number of which in the human ovum before maturation is thought to be sixteen. There is every reason lor believing that the chromatin is the bearer of whatever is inherited from the mother. The nucleus is limited by a nuclear membrane, and contains a strongly marked nucleolus, which has likewise retained its original name of germinal spot. There is probably no proper cell-wall, or vitelline membrane, such as is said to exist in many mammalian and other eggs. The ovum is, however, surrounded by a thick, tough, transparent membrane of ovarian origin, about 0.02 millimeter (rsViJ mcn) m thickness, and called the zona radiata or zona pellucida (Fig. 220, z). It is pierced by a multitude of fine lines radiating from the surface of the zona to the ovum ; these are thought to represent pores, to contain fine protoplasmic processes of the surrounding ovarian cells, and thus to serve as channels Cor the passage of nutriment to the egg. Between the zona radiata and the ovum a narrow space, the perivitelline space (x), exists. Attached to the outside of the zona ri-<>,i). The number of tetrads is hence one-half the number of original chromosomes, while the total number of chromosomes in the nucleus at this stage is double the original number. The nucleus moves from its position in the interior of the egg toward the sur- face, and the nuclear membrane begins to disappear. At the same time the two minute cytoplasmic structures, the centrosomes, which lie close beside the nucleus, separate and take up positions at a considerable distance apart from each other, in some cases even upon opposite sides of the nucleus. The 462 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. Fig. 221. Stages In the maturation of the ovum; diagrammatic (mainly from Wilson) : A, the orig- inal ovarian ovum; ». its nucleus, containing fbur chromosomes ; c, its double centrosome, surrounded by the attraction Bphere; in /.' much of the chromatin has begun to degenerate; the rest has become arranged Into two quadrupli ■ chromosomes or tetrads; the formation of the spindle and the asters has begun; in Cthe flrsl polar amphlaater, bearing the chromosomes, is completed ; in Dtheam- phiaster has become rotated and has travelled toward the Burface of the ovum; g. v, the degenerated remains of the nucleus; in E the division of the tetrads into double groups of chromosomes, or dyads, has begun, and the first polar body, p. 6«, is indicated ; In J" the first polar body, containing two dyads, has been extruded ; the formation ofthe Becond polar amphlaster has begun ; In G the firsi polar body is pre- paring to divide . polar amphlaster is fully funned ; in // the division ofthe dyads into single chromosomes In both the Brsl and the egg has begun, and the Becond polar body, p. 6», is in- dicated : in / the formation of the [Hilar bodies is completed ; j . the egg-nucleus, containing two small chromosomes, one-half the original Dumber. In fertilization the spermatozoon will bring in two addi- tional chromosomes, thus restoring the total number Oi lour. REPR OD UCTIOX. 453 substance lying between them — either the cytoplasmic network or the achro- matic substance of the nucleus — loses its reticular appearance, becomes fila- mentous, and arranges itself in the form of a spindle with the threads extend- ing from pole to pole (C, D). The groups of chromosomes become attached to the spindle threads midway between the poles. At each pole there may lie a centrosome, and about it the cytoplasm may become arranged in the form of a star, the aster, though these structures are not universal among species. The spindle with the two asters is known as the polar amphiastei', and the com- plicated structure seems to be formed, as in ordinary cell-division, for the sole purpose of dividing the nucleus into two portions. This is now per- formed (E) : each quadruple-group of chromosomes splits into two, and these, known as " double-groups," or " dyads," separate from each other and pass toward the poles of the spindle. The nucleus is thus divided into halves. While the division has been proceeding, the spindle has wandered halfway outside the egg, and, when it is completed, one of the resulting nuclear halves, comprising one-half of the full number of dyads, together with the centro- some and the aster, finds itself entirely extruded from the egg and lying within the perivitelline space. It is known as the first polar hod;/ ( /•'. p. 61). The diminished nucleus within the ovum proceeds at once to undergo a second mitotic division (G, H, I); each of the remaining dyads divides into two single chromosomes, which are separated from each other ; and a second polar body (p. b2), containing one-half the number of single chromosomes charac- teristic of the tissue-cells, is extruded. Apparently the two polar bodies are of no further use. In many animals the first divides into two, but sooner or later both degenerate and disappear. The remnant of the nucleus left within the egg, much reduced in size, wanders back to the interior. In the mam- mals no true tetrads are formed, and a considerable interval of time elapses between the formation of the two polar bodies, during which the sperma- tozoon enters the egg. But in them the process of maturation is the same in essence as in the lower animals. In all species the chromosomes are reduced to one-half the number belonging to the ovarian ovum ; in many species they are then resolved again into scattered chromatic substance. Tin1 nucleus develops a membrane and again enters the resting stage. It is known hence- forth as the egg-nucleus, or female pronucleus, and it await- the coming of the spermatozoon. According to most observers, its centrosome gradually degen- erates and disappears. Thus the curious process of maturation of the ovum is different in detail from that of maturation of the spermatozoon. In the latter the spermatocyte divides into four functional spermatozoa ; in the former the oocyte divides into two functionless polar bodies (or, by subdivision of the first, three, which have been called abortive c>j;ia)i tubes (Fig. 222, F. t), or oviducts, opens into the peritoneal cavity about one inch from the correspond- ing ovary. Around the opening is an expanded fringe of irregular processes, the fimbrke (/), one of which is attached to the ovary. The length of the tube is between three and four inches, and the opening into the uterus is extremely small. The chief structures in the walls of the oviducts that are of physio- logical interest are the double layer of plain muscle, an outer longitudinal and an inner circular coat, longitudinal fibres from which pass also into the fimbria? ; and the cilia with which the tube is lined throughout, and which are present also upon the inner side of the fimbria?. The direction of the ciliary movement is from the ovary toward the uterus. The primary function of the Fallopian tubes is to convey ova from the ovary to the uterus; they also con- vey spermatozoa in the reverse direction ; and within them the union of ovum and spermatozoon usually takes place. The mechanism of the receipt of the ovum by the tube is not fully under- stood. After ovulation the ovum is slightly adherent to the surface of the ovary by the agency of the viscid liquor foUkuU. It is possible, but it has not been proved, that in the human being, as has been seen in some animals, the expanded, fimbriated end of the Fallopian tube clasps the ovary when the egg is discharged. The passage of the ovum into the tube is probably brought about by the cilia lining the fimbria?. Once within the tube, the ciliarv action, assisted perhaps by contraction of the muscular fibres in the walls, carries the ovum slowly along toward and finally into the uterus. In -nine mammals the passage occupies three to five days; the time in woman is not definitely known, but i- thought to be from four to eight days. The Uterus. — The uterus (Fig. 222, u), or womb, receives the ovum from the Fallopian tube and passes it on, if unimpregnated, to the vagina; on the other hand, it receives from the vagina spermatozoa and transmits them to the Fallopian tubes ; it is the seat of the function of menstruation ; when impreg- nation has taken place, it retains and nourishes the growing embryo, and ulti- mately expels the child from the body. Its structure accords with these func- BEPBOD ( '( 'T/ON. 457 tions. Its thick walls consist largely of plain muscular tissue arranged roughly in the form of three indistinctly marked layers. Of these, the exter- nal and the middle coats are thin ; the fibres of the former are arranged in general longitudinally, those of the latter more circularly and obliquely. The third, most internal layer, which is regarded by some as a greatly hyper- trophied muscularis mucosae, forms the greater part of the uterine wall. Its fibres are arranged chiefly circularly ; toward the upper part they become trans- verse to the Fallopian tubes, and at the cervix longitudinal fibres lie within the circular ones. The individuality of the muscular layers and uniformity in the course of the fibres is largely interfered with by the numerous blood- vessels of the uterine walls. The uterus is lined by an epithelium composed of columnar ciliated cells, except in the lower half of the cervix, where a stratified non-ciliated epithelium exists. The direction of the ciliary movement in woman, as in other mammals, is toward the os uteri.1 The mucous membrane is thick, and contains very numerous, branching, tubular glands, which are lined by ciliated epithelium and have a tortuous course, terminating in the edge of the muscular layer. They secrete a viscid, mucous liquid. Between the glands are branched connective-tissue cells, which are not unlike the connective-tissue cells of embryonic structures, and wandering cells. Lymph-spaces and bl< >o< 1- capillaries exist. The development of the tissue goes on slowly up to the time of puberty, and, as we shall see, after puberty the mucous membrane is subject to constant change. Menstruation. — Except during pregnancy the most striking activities of the uterus are associated with that peculiar female function which, from its monthly periodicity, is called menstruation. The most obvious external fact of this phenomenon is the discharge every month of a bloody, mucous liquid through the vagina ; the most obvious internal facts are the bleeding and the degeneration and disappearance of a portion of the mucous membrane of the body of the uterus. This curious process, though having analogies in lower animals, occurs most markedly in the human female, and from before the time of Aristotle to the present, among both primitive and civilized races, its signifi- cance has been the cause of much speculation. The detailed phenomena of menstruation are not as well known as they should be. Experimentation is practically out of the question, and the opportunities of careful post-mortem study of normal healthy uteri at different stages are rare. The main facts arc as follows : Some days before the flow occurs the mucous membrane of the body of the uterus begins to thicken, partly by an active growth of its connective tissue elements and partly by an excessive filling of its capillaries and veins with blood. The cause of this swelling is not known. It continues until the membrane has doubled or trebled in thickness, and, according to some authori- ties, the uterine cavity becomes a mere slit between the walls. Then occurs an infiltration of blood-corpuscles and plasma, probably largely by diapedesis, although possibly assisted by rupture, through the walls of the swollen capil- 1 Hofmeier : Citilralhlnlf j'iir ihjuiikolixjic, lS'.t.'i, xvii. S. 7<>l. 458 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. laries into the connective-tissue spaces beneath the epithelial lining of the uterine wall. The epithelium is thus pressed up from beneath, and begins rapidly to undergo fatty degeneration in places, and to disappear. The immediate cause of the degeneration is nut definitely known. The connective- tissue elements and the upper portion of the glands are involved to some extent in the degenerative change. The capillaries, thus laid bare, burst, and the dark blood oozes forth and, mixed with disintegrated remains of the uterine tissues, with the mucous secretion of the uterus and the vagina, and with the escaped lymph, passes away, drop by drop, from the body. There is great difference of opinion as to the extent of the destruction of uterine tissue. On the one extreme side are those who claim that the loss of tissue is normally wholly trivial and secondary, the hyperemia and the bloody glandular discharge being the important events. Other authorities, equally extreme, have observed a disap- pearance of the whole mucous membrane except the deepest layers containing the bases of the glands ; this is probably pathological. From all the evidence an opinion inclining toward the former view seems most reasonable — namely, that usually and physiologically only the superficial portion of the mucous membrane disintegrates, and this only in spots.1 Differences in the amount undoubtedly occur. Occasionally it happens that the membrane, instead of disintegrating, comes away in pieces of considerable size. The term deddua nwnstrualis is applied to the lost coat. The flow continues upon an average four days or more. From observations upon 2080 American women Emmet2 finds the average duration of the flow at puberty to be 4.82 days, the average in later life 4.66 days. The amount of blood discharged can be determined only with great difficulty. It probably varies greatly, but is commonly estimated at from 100 to 200 cubic centimeters (4 to 5 ounces). The blood is slimy, with abundant mucus; usually it does not coagulate. Epithelium cells, red corpuscles, leucocytes, and detritus from the disintegrated tissues, occur in it. and it lias a characteristic odor. As the flow ceases a new growth of connective-tissue cells, capillaries, glands, and from the glands superficial epithelium, begins, and the mucous membrane is restored to its original amount. Whether a resting period follows before the succeeding tumefaction occurs is not definitely known, but it seems probable. The durations of the various steps in the uterine changes are not well known, and probably vary in individual cases. Minot3 suggests the following approximate times : Tumefaction of the mucosa, with accompanying structural changes 5 days. Menstruation proper 4 Restoration of the resting mucosa ... 7 Resting period 1- Total 28 days- The menstrua] changes in the uterus are accompanied by characteristic phenomena in other parts of the body. The Fallopian tubes are congested, lSee Westphalen : Archivjur Qynakologie, 1896, lii. S. 35 ; and Mandl: Ibid., 8. 557. 2T. A. Emmet- The Principle* and Practice of Gyrwecology, 1880, 2d ed. SC. S. Minot: Human Embryology, L892. BE PR OD UCTION. 459 and, according to some authorities, their mucous membrane degenerates and bleeds like that of the uterus. The ovaries are likewise congested. As has been stated, it is commonly believed, but not definitely proved, that ovulation accompanies each period. Frequent accompaniments are turgescence of the breasts, swelling of the thyroid and the parotid glands and the tonsils, conges- tion of the skin, dull complexion, tendency toward the development of pigment, and dark rings about the eyes. The skin and the breath may have a character- istic odor. In singers the voice is often impaired, which is one instance of a general nervous and muscular enervation. Mental depression often exists. Pain is a frequent accompaniment, and nervous and congestive pathological phenomena may, at times, become very pronounced. Recent work has shown that the various phenomena accompanying menstruation are evidences of a profound physiological change, with a monthly periodicity, that the female human organism undergoes, and of which the uterine changes are only a part. Thus, during the intermenstrual period there is a gradual increase of nervous tension and general mobility, of vascular tension manifested by turgescence of the blood-vessels, a gradual increase of nutritive activity manifested by increased production and excretion of urea and increased temperature, and a gradual increase of the heart's action in strength and rate.1 These various activities of the organism usually attain a maximum a few days before the menstrual flow begins and then undergo a rapid fall, which reaches a minimum toward the close of the flow ; a second lesser maximum may occur a few days after the flow ceases. All organic activities that have been carefully investi- gated show evidences of such a monthly rhythm. It is not known that the male possesses such a period. The first menstruation is usually regarded as the index of the oncoming of puberty or sexual maturity, and in temperate climates occurs usually at the age of fourteen to seventeen. Its onset is earlier in warm than in cold climates, in city than in country girls, and varies in time with food, growth, and environ- ment. Exceptionally menstruation may begin in infancy or later than puberty, and it has even been known to be wholly wanting in otherwise normal women. Normally, it ceases during pregnancy, and probably usually during lactation, although there are frequent exceptions to the latter rule. In nearly all cases complete removal of the ovaries puts an end to menstruation. Removal of the ovaries and Fallopian tubes diminishes the number of exceptional cases. The final cessation of menstruation, which is a gradual process extending over several months, usually marks the climacteric (menopause), or end of the sexual life, and occurs usually at the age of forty-five to forty-eight. Excep- tionally the flow may cease early in life or extend to extreme old age. ( 'omparative Physiology of Menstrua/ion. — The comparative physiology of menstruation, although it has been studied only incompletely in a few domesti- 1 Of. Mary Putnam Jacobi: "The Question of lust tor Women during Menstruation," Boylston Prize Essay, 1870; C. ReinI : Smnmlung kliniache Vortrdge, 1884, No. 243; 0. Ott: NouveUea archives a" obsUtrique et de gynecologic, 1890, v.; and A. E. Giles: Transactions of (he Obstetrical Society of London, 1897, xxxix. p. 115. 460 AN AMERICAN TEXT-HOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. cated animals and some monkeys,1 sheds some valuable light upon the phe- nomenon in woman. In animals lower than man, in a wild state, the desire arid power of reproduction are usually limited to seasonal periods. At such times conception is possible, and probably usually takes place. Such periods are known as "rut," "heat," and "oestrus." During the rest of the year sexual activities are in abeyance. Domestication, with its artificial condi- tions of regular food-supply, warmth, and care, has increased productiveness (Darwin) and rendered the reproductive periods more frequent. If impregna- tion be prevented, as is often the case in domesticated animals, the periods of "heat" appear for awhile with great frequency and regularity (monkey, mare, buffalo, zebra, hippopotamus, al intervals of four weeks; cow, three week-; sow, fifteen to eighteen days; sheep, two weeks; bitch, twelve to sixteen weeks). They arc characterized by general nervous excitement, desire and power of conception, congestion and swelling of the external genital organs, and a uterine discharge. The latter is scanty, mucous, and bloody, the amount of blood increasing in ascending the evolutionary scale. The histological processes occurring in the uterus have been studied carefully by Retterer in the dog and by Heape in the monkey. In the latter the proc- esses seem to be nearly identical with those of man. In the dog, growth and congestion of the mucosa occur, and are followed by rupture of the capil- laries, extravasation of blood, and degeneration of the tissues; but it is doubt- fid whether the epithelium is actually shed. It is generally believed that " heat " in the lower mammals is accompanied by ovulation. It is not neces- sarilv so in monkeys. The phenomena of " heat" are thus closely similar to those of human menstruation, the similarity being most marked in the monkeys. In addition to these more hidden phenomena there is present sexual desire, which in the human female is largely absent at such periods, although it may be pronounced just before and just after the actual flow. Theory of Menstruation. — The significance of menstruation is in great dis- pute. All modern theories agree in regarding it as associated in some way with the function of ohildbearing. The How was early believed to be a means employed by the body to get rid of a plethora of nutriment. This was followed by the well-known hypothesis, put forward especially by Pfliiger (1865). According to this hypothesis,2 the menstrual bleeding and the uterine denudation occur for the purpose of providing a fresh uterine surface to which tli'- egg, if impregnated, can readily attach itself, just as, in graft- ing, the gardener provides a wounded surface upon which the young scion is set, or, in uniting two membrane-covered tissues, the surgeon first wounds or freshens their surfaces. This conception of menstruation i> not now commonly accepted. Pfliiger regards the mechanism of the uterine process to be as fol- lows -. The constant growth of the ovarian cells and the consequent swelling of 1 Cf. A. Wiltshire: British Medical Journal, March, 1883; I-'.. Retterer: Comptea rendm (n), the clitoris, which is the diminutive homologue of the penis of the male, and the hymen (//), or perforated curtain that guards the entrance to the vagina and is usually ruptured at the time of the first coition. The vulva receives the openings of the vagina, the urethra (u), and the ducts of Bartholin's glands. Its parts are capable of turgidity through its rich vas- cular supply, and perform minor ill-defined, adaptive, and stimulating func- tions in copulation. Their surface is covered by mucous membrane which is moistened and lubricated by a secretion from numerous mucous follicles, seba- ceous glands, and the glands of Bartholini. The latter are comparable to Cowper's glands of the male and secrete a viscid liquid. The Mammary Glands. — The mammary glands, being active only during the period of lactation, may best be studied in connection with that function (see Vol. I., p. 261). Internal Secretion. — A priori, the reproductive organs can scarcely be regarded as organs that are quiescent during the greater part of life and pas- 1 "The largest number [of human births] almost always falls in the month of February, .... corresponding to conceptions in May and June Observations tend to show the largest number of conceptions in Sweden falling in June; in Holland and France, in May-June; in Spain. Austria, and Italy, in May; in Greece, in April. That is, the farther south the earlier the spring and the earlier the conceptions.'' — Mayo-Smith : Statistics and Sociology, 1895. REPRODUCTION. 463 sively await the reproductive act. The view that they are more than this is supported by some, although slight, experimental evidence. Notwithstanding the fact that removal of the testis or the ovary in adult life is often unaccom- panied by great somatic changes, the profound effects of early castration upon development, in both the male and female, show that upon the presence of the sexual organs depends the appearance of many of the secondary sexual cha- racters— characters which apparently are independent of those organs, and yet of themselves distinguish the individual as specifically masculine or feminine. The mode of dynamic reaction of the sexual organs upon the other organs can at present be little more than hinted at. It is entirely probable that such reaction is either nervous or chemical, or perhaps it is both combined. Regarding the former little is known. Regarding the latter certain facts point to a possible normal and constant contribution of specific material by the reproductive glands to the blood or lymph, and thus to the whole body. Such a process is spoken of as internal secretion. This subject is discussed more fully in Vol. I. p. 273. D. The Reproductive Process. Thus far attention has been given to the general functions of the repro- ductive organs. We come now to the special phenomena connected with the reproductive process itself, and have to trace the history of the spermatozoon, the ovum, and the embryo. It should be borne clearly in mind that the essential part of the reproductive process is the fusion of the nuclei of the two germ-cells. Investigation is making it more and more probable that the spermatozoon and the ovum, although so different in appearance and general behavior, are fundamentally and in origin both morphologically and physi- ologically equivalent cells. In the processes of their growth and maturation they are secondarily modified, the one into an active locomotive body, the other into a passive nutritive body. The modifications in both are confined, how- ever, to the cell-protoplasm (cytoplasm and centrosome) ; the essential parts, the nuclei, remain unmodified and both morphologically and physiologically equivalent down to the time of their fusion in the process of fertilization. The many and complex details of the reproductive process exist for the sole purpose of bringing together these two minute masses of chromatin.1 Copulation. — Copulation is the act of sexual union, and has for its object the transference of the semen from the genital passages of the male to those of the female. It is preceded by erection of the penis and turgidity of the organs of the vulva. These latter occurrences are in the main vascular phenomena, and are brought about by a distention of the cavernous spaces of the erectile tissues with blood. The vascular phenomena are, however, accompanied by complex nervous and muscular activities. As regards the penis, the arteries supplying the organ relax and allow blood to flow in quantity to the corpora cavernosa and the corpus spongiosum. Simultaneous relaxation of the smooth 1 Compare Th. Boveri : " Befrnchtunp;," Merkel und Bonnet's Ergebnwe der Atiatomie und Entwickelungsgeschichte, 1892, i. 1 1 i I AN AM EB ICA X TEXT-B O OK OF PHYSIOL O G ) '. muscle fibres scattered throughout the trabecular framework of the corpora increases the capacity of the blood-spaces. Furthermore, the ischio-cavernosus {erector penis) and bulbo-cavernosus muscles contract aud compress the proximal or bulbous ends of the corpora and the outgoing veins. The result of this combined muscular relaxation and contraction is a free entrance of blood into and a difficult exit from the vascular spaces ; this leads to a swelling and distention -which aid further in compressing the venous outlets and, being limited by the tough, fibrous tunics of the corpora, result in making the organ stiff", hard, erect in position, and well adapted to its specific function. During the process of erection the cresla of the urethra or capvi galfinaginis, which is an elevation extending from the cavity of the bladder into the prostatic por- tion of the urethra and containing erectile tissue, becomes turgid and, by the aid of the contraction of the sphincter urethrce, effectually closes the passage into the bladder. Erection is a complex reflex act, the centre of which lies in the lumbar spinal cord and may be aroused to activity by nervous impulses coming from different directions. Impulses may originate in the walls of the ducts of the testis from the pressure of the contained semen or in the penis from external stimulation of the nerve-endings in the skin, in both cases passing along the sensory nerves of the organs to the spinal centre ; or they may originate in the brain and pass downward through the cord, the impulses in this case corresponding to sexual emotions. The centrifugal paths for the arteries are along the nervi erigentcs, which are true vaso-dilator nerves, and in the mammals, where experiment has proved their existence, pass from the spinal cord along the posterior lumbar (monkey) or anterior sacral (monkey, dog, cat) nerves to their arterial distribution. The ischio- and bulbo-caverno- sus muscles are under the control of their motor nerve supply, consisting of branches of the perineal nerve. In the female, anatomists recognize the homologues of the male erectile parts as follows : the clitoris with its corpora cavernosa and glans as the horno- logue of the penis, the two bulbi vestibuli as that of the bulb of the corpus spongiosum, the pars intermedia perhaps as that of the corpus spongiosum itself, and the erector clitoridis muscle as the homologue of the erector ])enis (iscfiio-cavernosus). The mechanism of erection is similar to that in the male, and the result is a considerable degree of firmness in the external genital organs. The sexual excitement attendant upon copulation is usually much greater in man than in woman, and culminates in the sexual orgasm, when the emis- sion of semen from the penis into the vagina occurs. It will be remembered that the prepared semen is stored in the ducts of the testes. The discharge of the fluid is a muscular act which begins probably in the vasa efferentia and the canal of the epididymis, and sweeps along the powerful muscular walls of the vasa deferentia in the form of a series of peristaltic waves. The Beminal vesicles also contract, and the mixed liquid and spermatozoa are poured through the ejaculatory ducts into the prostatic portion of the urethra. The muscles of the prostate expel the prostatic fluid and help to pass the semen RE PR OD UCTION. 465 onward. The glands of Cowper possibly add their contribution. But the final urethral discharge is effected especially by powerful rhythmic contractions of the already partially contracted striped muscles, viz. the ischio- and bulbo- cavernosi, the constrictor urethrce, and probably the anal muscles, the result of the complex series of actions being to expel the semen with some force into the upper part of the vagina close to the os uteri. Ejaculation is a reflex act. The centre lies in the lumbar spinal cord ; the centripetal nerves are the sen- sory nerves of the penis, stimulation of the glans being especially effective; the centrifugal nerves are the nerves to the various muscles. In the female during ejaculation the glands of Bartholini pour out a mucous liquid upon the vulva. There is possibly a downward movement of the uterus, brought about by contraction of its round ligaments and accompanied perhaps by a contrac- tion of the uterine walls themselves. Bat all muscular and erectile activity, as well as sexual passion, is usually less pronounced in woman than in man. Locomotion of the Spermatozoa. — The union of the spermatozoon and the ovum probably takes place usually in the Fallopian tube not far from its ovarian end, and to this place the spermatozoa at once proceed. Their mode of entrance into the uterus is not wholly clear ; it is quite generally believed, but without conclusive experimental proof, that relaxation of the uterus im- mediately after copulation exerts a suction upon the liquid which aids in its passage through the os and the cervix. It is possible that active contraction of the vaginal walls assists. Spermatozoa have been found in the uterus a half hour after coition.1 The main agency in the locomotion of the sper- matozoa through the body of the uterus and the Fallopian tubes, and prob- ably also from the vagina into the uterus, is the spontaneous movement of the spermatozoa themselves. By the lashing of their tails they wriggle their way over the moist surface, being stimulated to lively activity probably by the opposing ciliary movements in the epithelium lining the passages. Kraft2 has shown in the rabbit that, when spermatozoa in feeble motion are placed upon the inner surface of the oviduct, not only are they thrown into active contrac- tions, but they move against the ciliary movement, i. e. up the oviduct. The capacity of the male cells thus to respond by locomotion in the opposite direc- tion to the stimulating influence of the ciliary cells over which they have to pass, is an interesting adaptation. Probably this is the directive agency that enables the spermatozoa to follow the right path to the ovum, while the ovum, being in itself passive, is by the same ciliary movement brought toward th<> active male cell. The time occupied in the passage of the spermatozoa is un- known in the human female, but is probably short ; in the rabbit spermatozoa have been known to reach the ovary within two and three-quarter hours after copulation. As has been seen, spermatozoa arc probably capable of living within the genital passages for several days, when, if ovulation has not taken place, they perish. If, however, an ovum appears, they at •<■ approach and surround it in great numbers, being apparently attracted t<> it in some myste- 1 Schuworski : Abstract in Monatsschrift fwr Oeburlshulfe nu). This body has the conventional nuclear structurt — namely, an achr atic network with the chromatic reticulum mingled with it — and the whole is covered by a nuclear membrane. From the observations of Van Beneden, Riickert,2 Zoja,3 and others, it seems to be a fact that the male and the female chromosomes do not fuse together, but remain distinct from each other, per- haps throughout all the tissue-cells. The chromosomes, it will be perceived, are now restored to the original nber presenl in either germ-cell before its maturation, hence in the human being perhaps sixteen, one-half of them 1 \V. Pfeffer: Untersuchungen au&dem Botanischen Tn&titutzu Tubingen, 1884, i. -J. Riickert: Archiv fiir mikroskopische Anatomic, 1895, xlv. K. Zoja: Anatomischer Anzeiger, 1896, xi. BE PR OD UCTION. 467 bavins: come, however, from the mule cell and one-half from the female cell. On the commonly accepted theory that they constitute the hereditary .-ub- stance, the first segmentation-nucleus contains within itself potentially all the inherited qualities of the future individual. While the head of the spermatozoon is making its way through the sub- stance of the egg there appears beside it a minute cytoplasmic body, the centrosame, and around the latter cytoplasmic filaments arrange themselves in the form of a star, the whole body being known as the sperm-aster (Fig. 223, b). We have previously recognized such a structure in the ovum at the time of maturation, and have found it functional in the formation of the polar bodies; after maturation it disappears. The sperm-aster accompanies the sperm-nucleus, becomes gradually enlarged, and finally comes to lie, a large and prominent body, beside the segmentation-nucleus. The origin of its centrosome has been greatly disputed. Some investigators maintain that Fig. 223.— Stages in the fertilization of the egg (after Wilson). The drawings were made from sections of the eggs of the sea-urchin, Toxopneustes variegatus, Ag. a. The surface of the egg has become elevated to form e, the entrance-cone for the spermatozoon ; the head (ft) and the middle-piece (m) of the latter have entered the egg. B. Five minutes after entrance of the spermatozoon. The head, now the male pronucleus, has rotated 180 degrees, and has travelled deeper into the ovum. The cytoplasm of the latter has become arranged in a radiate manner about the middle-piece of the spermatozoon, now the centrosome, to form the sperm- aster; the egg-nucleus, now the female pronucleus, is approaching the sperm-nucleus; its chromatin forms an irregular reticulum. it is formed anew in the egg; but the prevalent opinion at present seems to be that it comes from the spermatozoon in immediate relation to the middle- piece, and hence is exclusively of male origin. There results from fertilization, it is perceived, a single cell complete in all its essential parts. This is the starting-point of the new individual. A pause or resting period usually follows fertilization, ami then growth begins. Segmentation. — The process of growth is a complex process of repealed cell-division, increase in bulk, morphological differentiation, and physiological division of labor. Cell-division is largely, if not wholly, indirect or mitotic. The term seg- mentation, or cleavage, of the ovum is conveniently applied to the 6rs1 few divisions, although the details of segmentation are not different fundamen- tally from those manifested later in the division of more specialized cells. Each division may be resolved into three definite acts, which, however, overlap each other in time. The first act is characterized by the appearance of two centrosomes, each with its astral rays, in place of the one already }»;> AN AMERICAN TENT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. existing (Fig. 224, c). The two take positions beside the nucleus (Fig. 224, d) and await the time when they can exert their specific function. AVe have spoken of the difference of opinion regarding the origin of the original cen- fcrosome of fertilization. The origin of the two centrosonies present in seg- mentation has likewise been disputed. The question is of considerable the- oreticaJ interest in connection with the problem of the physical basis of Pig 224.— Stages in the fertilization oJ the egg (continued from Fig. 223). minuti - after entrance of the spermatozoon. The male and the female pronuclei have met in a r the centre of the egg and the fusion 1ms begun ; the former has become enlarged and its chromatin has become loosely reticulated. The sperm-aster has become enormously enlarged. The single centro- some has been divided into two, which lie upon either side of the Bperm-nucleus. d. "-till later after entrance of the spermatozoon. The two pronuclei have united to form the firsl segmentation-nucleus. The sperm-aster bas become divided into two asters, which have moved to opposite poles of the nucleus. The i gg is nov ready to undergo segmentation. inheritance. Certain observers have claimed that the centrosomes have a double origin, one being derived from the male and one from the female germ-cell. Upon this theory sexuality is shown by the cytoplasmic centro- somes as well as by the nuclear chromosomes, and the inference is possible that cytoplasm, as well as nucleus, transmits hereditary qualities. This double origin of the centrosomes is not supported by trustworthy evidence. Oilier observers, following Boveri, maintain that the centrosome of the sperma- BE PROD UCTION. 469 / m< tozoon divides into the two segmentation-centrosomes, the latter hence being exclusively of male origin. Still others believe that the sperm- centrosome disappears, its place being taken by two new ccntro- somes derived from the cyto- plasm of the egg. The evi- dence available at present does not allow a decision to be made between these two latter views.1 According to both of them, however, the cleavage-eentro- somes are not male and female, and cannot be regarded as bearers of inherited character- istics. These observations not only allow, but tend to strengthen, the prevailing view of the exclusive hereditary role of the nucleus. (See below under Heredity, p. 493.) The second act of segmenta- tion is more complicated than the first, and consists of a halv- ing of the nucleus. The nuclear membrane gradually disappears. The achromatic network resolves itself into long cytoplasmic fila- ments arranged in the form of a spindle, and meeting at the two ccntrosomes (Fig. 225, a). The spindle, ccntrosomes, and asters form the body known as the amphiaster. The chromatic sub- stance becomes changed into the definite rod-like; chromosomes, which are collected in the equa- tag as delicate filaments extending in the f< . , « , • n l from poll' tn polr. tonal zone ot the spindle, ana B, Each chromosome has become split into two. and the constitute the equatorial plate, latter are being pulled toward the poles. * * c. The divergence oi the chromosomes has ceased and Each chromosome proceeds to the Latter are becoming converted Into vesicular masses split lengthwise and the two ,"'si'''' the centrosomes. The spindle is becoming resolved '' & * Into ordinary cytoplasm, halves move toward the t\v<> ccntrosomes (Fig. 22o, b). The cause of this movement is not known. The 'For a critical review of iliis and oilier problems in fertilization and segmentation Bee E. B. Wilson: The Cell in Development and Inheritance, 1900, 2d ed., New York. Fig 225.— Stages in the segmentation of the egg (after Wilson). The drawings were made from sections of eggs of the sea-urchin, Toxopneuxtes variegatus, kg. a. The nuclear membrane has disappeared within the nucleus a distinction between the chromatic and the achro- matic substance has been made, the former existing as clearly defined chromosomes aggregated in the centre to form the equatorial plate, the achromatic substance exist nn of a spindle 470 AN AMERICA* TEXT- BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. original idea of Van Beneden,1 that the astral raws are contractile and mechanically pull apart the half-chromosomes, is supported by considerable lait unconvincing evidence. The idea appears to be growing that by reason of chemical changes taking place in the centrosomes the halt-chromosomes are attracted t<> the two poles of the spindle.2 Strasburger3 suggests that this attractive influence is chemotaxis. In the process of division each nuclear half obtains half of the original male and half of the original female chromatin, and hence contains inherited potentialities of both parents. After division each half gradually assumes the structure of a typical resting nucleus with its accompanying aster (Fig. 226). The third act of segmentation consists of a simple division of the cytoplasm into two ecpial parts, the separation taking place along the plane of nuclear / / I Jrar ''<7\~* \ / \ i i i / Fir,. 226.— Stages in the segmentation of the egg (continued from Fig. 226). d. The vesicular chromatic masses have become converted into two typical resting nuclei, each with n chromatic network. The single aster, formerly connected with each nuclear mass, bas become divided into two, which have taken positions at opposite poles of the nuclei. The division of the cytoplasm is complete, and the two resulting cells, or blastomeres, are resting, preparatory to a second division in a plane at right angles to that of the first. division (Fig. 226, i>). Each part contains one of the new nuclei, and the result of the firsl division is the existence of two cells, two blastomeres, in place of the one fertilized ovum. The beginning of differentiation is often shown even as early as this, for one blastomere is often somewhat larger and less granular than the other. Each blastomere proceeds now to divide by a similar mitotic process into two. the resull being four in all, and by subsequent divisions, eight, sixteen, and more, the division- not proceeding, however, with mathematical rcgu- laritv. By such repeated mitotic processes the original fertilized ovum becomes a mas- of small and approximately similar cells, the morula, from which by continued increase in the number of the cells, morphological differ- entiation, and physiological division of labor, the embryo with all its functions is destined to be built up. 'Van Beneden: Arclt'n-rs ck Iliolat/k, iss.'j, iv. 1 - v . Biitechli: Verh. NalurhwL med. Ver. Heidelberg, 1891 ; and E. B. "Wilson, op. cit. 'Strasburger: Analomi&cher Anzeiger, 1893, viii. REPRODUCTION. All Polyspermy. — It happens occasionally that two or more spermatozoa enter the ovum ; such a phenomenon is known as dispermy or 'polyspermy, according to the number of entering sperms. Each sperm with its nucleus and centro- somc becomes a male pronucleus and proceeds to conjugate with the female pronucleus. In the case of dispermy the one female and the two male pro- nuclei i'u^c together; each centrosome gives place as usual to two, making four in all, which take up a quadrilateral position about the firsl segmenta- tion-nucleus; the chromatic figure consists of two crossed spindles; and the egg segments at once into four instead of two blastomeres. Analogous phe- nomena result from more complex cases of polyspermy. In such double- or multi -fertilized eggs development may proceed to some distance, but typical larval forms are not produced, and death occurs early. During cleavage the ovum proceeds, after the manner of the non-fertilized ovum, slowly along the Fallopian tube and enters the uterus. Unlike the non- fertilized ovum, however, the morula is not cast out of the body, but remains and undergoes further development. The morphological development of the embryo in utero does not fall within the scope of the present article. Some attention may, however, be given to the immediate environment of the develop- ing child and its relations to the maternal organism. Decidua Graviditatis. — While the segmentation of the ovum is proceed- ing within the Fallopian tube, the uterus prepares for the future guest by begin- ning to undergo a profound change, probably being stimulated to activity re- flexly by centripetal impulses originating in the walls of the tube through con- tact with the ovum. This change comprises an enlargement of the whole uterus and a great and rapid growth in thickness of its mucosa and its muscular coat. At first the alterations are not unlike the phenomena of growth pre- ceding the menstrual flow, but, as they proceed, they become much more pro- found than those. The supply of blood to the walls is greatly increased, the vessels forming large irregular sinuses within the mucosa. The supply of lymph is increased. The glands become tortuous and dilated into flattened cavernous spaces, and their walls atrophy, the epithelium breaking down except in their deepest parts. The mucosa is thus converted into a spongy tissue, the frame- work of which contains numerous large irregular cells, derived probably from the original connective tissue and called decidual cells. The musculature is greatly thickened by an increase, partly in number and partly in size, of its constituent fibres, and the nerve-supply is Increased. These general structural changes proceed through the early part of gestation and arc accompanied In- special changes to be discussed later. It is not definitely known how far the alterations have gone before the advent of the segmented ovum into the uterus. With the latter instead of the unimpregnated ovum present in the Fallopian tube, the hypertrophied uterine mucosa docs not break away as in menstrua- tion, but remains, and henceforth is called the decidua graviditatis, special names being given to special parts. Entering the uterus, the ovum attaches itself in an unknown manner to the wall of the womb. The part of the mucous 472 AN AMERICAN TEXT-HOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. membrane that forms its bed is henceforth known as the decM.ua serotina; as the seat of the future placenta, it is physiologically the most interesting and important portion of the uterine mucosa. The surrounding cells and tissues are stimulated to active proliferation and grow around and over the ovum, completely covering it with a layer, the decidua rejiexa. The remainder of the uterine lining membrane constitutes the decidua vera. Between the reflexa and the vera is the uterine cavity. At first thickened, the reflexa later thins away as the embryo grows, and approaches close to the vera; finally it touches the latter, and the original cavity of the body of the uterus becomes oblit- erated. By the sixth month the reflexa disappears, either coalescing with the vera or undergoing total degeneration (Minot). During the latter half of gestation the vera itself thins markedly. This atrophy of the comparatively unimportant reflexa and vera, in contrast to the placental hypertrophy of the serotina, is interesting. The arrangement of the parts is well shown in the accompanying illustration (Fig. 227). The Fetal Membranes. — The segmented ovum absorbs nutriment at first directly from its surrounding maternal tissues, and later through the mediation of the placenta. Its growth and cell-division are active, and it increases in size and complexity. It early takes the form of a generalized vertebrate em- bryo, and by the fortieth day begins to assume distinctly human characteristics. It becomes surrounded early by the fetal membranes, which are two in num- ber, the amnion and the chorion or, as it is usually called in other vertebrates, false amnion. The amnion is a thin, transparent, non-vascular membrane imme- diately surrounding the embryo (Fig. 227). In origin a derivative of the embry- onic somatopleure, later it becomes completely separated from the body of the embryo. The space enclosed by the amnion, the amniotic cavity, within which the embryo lies, is traversed by the umbilical cord and contains a serous liquid, the liquor amnii. This Liquid, highly variable in quantity, averages at full term nearly a liter (If pints). It has in general the composition of a serous liquid, it contains between 1 and 2 per cent, of solids, consisting of proteids (0.06-0.7 per cent.), mucin, a minute and variable quantity of urea, and inor- ganic salts. Its origin, whether from the fetus, especially from the fetal kidneys, or from the mother, has been much discussed. It may possibly come in small part from the former, but its chief origin is doubtless by trans- udation from the maternal blood, as is indicated by the ready appearance within the amniotic cavity of solutions injected into the maternal veins, and the fact that the amniotic Liquid of diabetic women contains sugar. It bathes the entire surface of the embryonic body, and is, moreover, apparently swal- lowed into the stomach, as the presence of fetal hairs and epidermal scales within the alimentary canal attests. Its chief functions appear to be those of protecting the fetus from sudden shocks and from pressure, maintaining a constant temperature, and supplying the fetal body with water. The pro- teid possibly confers upon it a very slight nutritive value, and the minute quantity of urea is perhaps indicative of an unimportant excretory function of the fital kidneys. As growth proceeds, the amnion expands and becomes loosely attached to the outer fetal membrane, the chorion. REPRODUCTIOX. 473 The chorion (Fig. 227), or false amnion, is formed simultaneously with the true amnion, and like it from somatopleure. It is a thickened vascular mem- brane, completely surrounding the amnion with the contained embryo. Be- tween it and the amnion there is at first a considerable space, traversed by the umbilical cord and filled with the chorionic fluid (which is probably of the same general nature as the amniotic fluid). But later this space is obliterated Decidua serotina. Chorion frondosum. —Mucous plug within cervical canal. Fig. 227.— Diagram of the human uterus nt the seventh or eighth week of pregnancy modified from Allen Thompson). The fetal villi are shown growing into the Binuses of the decidua serotina and the decidua reflexa; in the latter they are becoming atrophied. They are marked by the black fetal vessels, whiehcanbe traced backward along the umbilical conl to the embryo. The placenta comprises the decidua serotina and the chorion frondosum. by the enlargement of the amnion. Externally the chorion presents, at first, a shaggy appearance due to the existence of very numerous columnar pro- cesses, called vUU, extending outward in all directions and joining by their tips the decidua serotina and the decidva reflexa. Later the villi are aborted except in the region of the serotina, where they become more prominent and constitute an important part of the placenta. The blood-vessels of the chorion 474 AX AMIUUCAX TEXT-HOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. Amnion. i i 'horion. i are fetal vessels coming from the embryonic structure, the allantois. They comprise the branches and uniting capillaries of the two allantoic or umbilical arteries, and the one (at first two) allantoic or umbilical vein. They are especially well developed within the villi. As growth proceeds, the chorion comes into close contact with the deeidua reflexa, and, as the latter disappears, with the deeidua vera; this portion of it is called chorion Iceve. In the region of the deeidua serotina it enters into the formation of the placenta, and is here called chorion frondosum. The Placenta. — The placenta (Fig. --7), or organ of attachment of mother and fetus, is a disk-shaped body, approximately 20 centimeters (7—8 inches) in diameter, attached to the inner surface of the uterine wall, usually either upon the dorsal or the ventral side, and connected by the umbilical cord with the navel of the ictus. It consists of a maternal part, the modified deeidua serotina, and a fetal part, the modified chorion, intimately united together. The modifica- tions of the serotina consist of a degeneration of the superficial layers of the mucosa, especially of the epithelium and the glands, and the development of very large irregular sinuses at the surface, into which the uterine arteries and veins freely open. It is a disputed question among histologists whether the sinuses an- maternal or fetal in origin, or really spaces between maternal and fetal tissues. The modifications of the chorion con- sist of a great increase in length and complexity of branching of the villi, a greai development of their contained blood-vessels, and a linn attachment of their tips to the uneven surface of the serotina, so that their branches come to float Pig. 228. Diagram ol the placenta (Schafer) : s, pla- centa) sinuses, into which project the fetal villi, con- freely within the uterine sinuses taining tin- nil fetal > otina; m. 1 . i 1 .1 1 ■ • 11 1 spongy layer, and m, muscular layer, of the uterus; a, :iml to De bathed in uterine blood uterine artery, and v, uterine vein, opening into tin- (Fig. 228). The analogy between placental sinuses. . ,. . , .... . the mammalian placental villi and the gills of a fish, also highly vascular and floating in liquid, is striking. We shall see later that the analogy is not only morphological but also physiological, inasmuch as the villi have important respiratory functions. The bulk of the placenta is this intravillous portion, of spongy consistence, REPRODUCTION. 475 comprising the maternal sinuses permeated by the fetal villi ; this is in con- tact upon the fetal side with the thin unmodified chorion covered within by the amnion, and upon the maternal side with the thin relatively unmodified serotina covered without by the uterine muscle. The pure maternal blood brought by the uterine arteries moves slowly through the sinuses and retiree by the uterine veins; the fetal blood is propelled by the fetal hear! along the umbilical cord within the allantoic arteries and through the villous capillaries, and returns by the allantoic vein. The two kinds of blood never mix, but are always separated by the thin capillary walls and their thin villous invest- ment of connective tissue and epithelium. Thus the anatomical conditions for ready diffusion are present, and this is the chief means of transfer of nutriment and oxygen from mother to child, and of wastes from child to mother. The physiological role of the placenta is, therefore, an all-important and complicated one. The placenta is, technically, the nutritive organ of the embryo. Nutrition of the Embryo. — We have seen that a fundamental and most striking difference between the minute human ovum and the large e probable that, with the uterus undoubtedly irritable, in different 1 J. Veit : Midler's Handbuch der QeburtohiUfe, 1888, 1. REPRODUCTION. 479 cases different stimuli act to inaugurate the process of birth, and a priori several of the above causes seem not improbable ones. Parturition in General. — Parturition, birth, or labor, is the process of expulsion of the developed embryo, the membranes, and the placenta from the body of the mother. It is executed by contraction of the muscles of the so- called upper segment of the uterus and those of the abdominal walls. The lower segment pf the uterus, comprising approximately that portion of the body lying; below the attachment of the peritoneum, the cervix, the vagina, and the vulva, are largely, if not wholly, passive in parturition. The obstet- ricians have found it convenient to divide labor into three stages, although physiologically these are not sharply differentiated from each other. The first stage is characterized by the dilatation of the oh uteri, the second by the expul- sion of the fetus, the third by the expulsion of the after-birth. The customary position of the fetus within the uterus at the end of pregnancy is that in which the head is downward or nearest the os, the back toward the ventral and left side of the mother, and the arms and legs folded upon the trunk. First Stage of Labor. — For several weeks toward the close of pregnancy there are occasional periods when rhythmic muscular contractions pass over the uterine walls. These are mostly painless, and apparently are not in themselves of special functional importance. The first stage of labor is ushered in by various phenomena, prominent among which are an increase in the intensity of the contractions, their painfulness, and their frequency and continuance. In women they are confined practically to the upper segment of the uterus and its attached ligaments, ceasing at a circular ridge that projects inward and is called the "contraction ring." For some reason, at present disputed, the lower segment of the uterus, and the cervix, arc passive. The contractions are probably peristaltic in character, as in lower animals. Schatz1 has graphi- cally recorded the uterine movements by means of a bladder filled with water and introduced into the uterus. During the earlier part of parturition the contraction- gradually increase in intensity up to a maximum which they then maintain. Their rhythm is somewhat irregular \ the duration of each contraction averages about one minute, and a pause, which ensues between suc- cessive contractions, extends from one and one-half to several minutes. The relaxation of the muscle-fibres during the period of rest is incomplete, the result being that the fibres enter gradually into a tonieally contracted state. Each contraction is accompanied by a pain, localized in the early pari of labor in the uterus alone, but later extending outward, upward into the abdomen, and downward into the thighs. The pains of labor vary greatly in intensity in individuals, but are usually more intense during the firsl gestation than during later ones. They are due chiefly to direct mechanical stimulation of the sensory uterine and other nerves by compression, tension, and even laceration. 1 V. Schatz : Archiv fur Uyuil koloyir, INN") S(i, xwii. < ompare O. Schaeffer : Experimentelle Untersuchungen iiier die Wehentkatigkeii den menschlichen Uterus, auagefiihrt miitelst einer neuen Pelolte iokI eines neuen Kymoyraphionf Berlin, 1896; abstract in Centralblatt fur Gynalcologie, 1896, xx. 8. 85. 480 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OE PHYSIOLOGY. As a result of the tonic contraction of the uterine walls, gradually increas- ing with each new peristaltic wave, the uterus becomes gradually narrower in diameter and longer, and the walls press more and more firmly upon the bag of amniotic liquid containing the embryo. Schatz finds that the uterine pres- sure under the uterine contractions rarely reaches and never exceeds 100 milli- meters of mercury. The direction of least resistance to this pressure lies along the cervical canal, the walls of which do not take part in the uterine labor. With each succeeding contraction this canal is forced wider open and the uterine contents are pressed tightly downward and into the eervix. The head of the embryo is preceded by a bulging portion of the membrane, filled with liquid and forming a distinct bladder-like advance-guard. This bag appears at the os uteri, its contents increase under the increasing pressure, and in the majority of cases, when the os is fully expanded, it bursts and allows the amniotic liquid to escape to the exterior. In some cases the rupture is delayed until the sec- ond stage of labor, and rarely the child is born with the membranes intact. Second Stage of Labor. — The uterine contractions frequently eease for a period following the rupture of the membrane. They then begin anew with increased force, and are accompanied by a new feature, namely, analogous vigorous rhythmic contractions of the muscles of the abdominal walls. These, following deep inspiration and accompanied by forced attempts at expiration with a closed glottis, diminish the longitudinal and the lateral diameters of the abdominal cavity, compress the abdominal organs, and help to augment greatly the uterine pressure. At the beginning of the second stage the force of the contractions is expended mainly upon the head of the embryo, which lies like a plug in the cervical canal. This is squeezed gradually through the os into the vagina, followed by the more easily passing trunk and limbs. The con- tractions are frequent, vigorous, and painful, the pains reaching a maximum as the sensitive vulva is put upon the stretch and traversed. The vertex is usually presented first to the exterior, the head and body following as the suc- cessive contractions of the maternal muscles develop sufficient power to over- come the resistance offered to their passage by the surrounding walls. In the human female the vaginal muscles do not appear to engage in the expel- ling act, the uterine and the abdominal muscles alone sufficing and finally forcing the child wholly outside the mother's body. In this gradual manner, painful and dangerous alike to mother and child, the maternal organism forces the offspring to forsake its sheltering and nutritive walls and begin its inde- pendent existence. Third Stage of Labor. — During the later expulsive contractions of the second stage the placenta, being greatly folded by the diminution in the uterine surface of attachment, is loosened from the uterine wall by a rupture taking place through the Loose tissue in the region of the blood-sinuses. The child, when born, is joined to the loosened placenta by the umbilical cord, until the latter is tied and cut by the obstetrician. The muscular contractions, now almost painless, continue through the third stage, and the placenta is torn from its attachment, everted, and carried gradually outward. The lining REPRODUCTION. 4*1 membrane of the uterus from the placenta outward and for a considerable depth is gradually torn free from the deeper parts through the spongy layer, and with the attached chorion and amnion follows the placenta. As a rule, this after-birth appears at the vulva within fifteen minutes after the expulsion of the child; it consists of the placenta, the amnion, the chorion, the deddua reflexa, and a considerable portion of the . wli. • ( '. S. Minot : Human Embryology, Is'.1'-!. 488 AN AMERICA X TEXT-BOOK OE PHYSIOLOGY height occurs, while from the accumulation of fat the weight usually rises markedly up to the fiftieth or the sixtieth year. One of the most interesting results revealed by statistics is the relative growth of the two sexes. From birth up to about the age of ten or twelve, boys show a slight and increasing preponderance over girls, but the two curves are nearly parallel. The prepu- bertal acceleration of growth in girls, however, precedes that of boys, and is even accompanied by some check in the male growth, with the result that between the ages of twelve and fifteen girls are actually heavier and taller than boys. This fact, first pointed out in 1872 by Bowditch1 from observa- Fig. 229.— Diagram showing increase of stature and weight of both sexes, as determined by the Anthropo- metric ( lommittee of the British Association.2 tions on several thousand Boston school children, has been abundantly con- tinued by Pagliani in Italy, Key in Sweden, Schmidt in Germany, Porter in St. Louis, and others. At about fifteen years boys again take the lead and maintain it throughout life. Boys grow most rapidly at sixteen, girls at thir- teen or fourteen, years of age; the former attain their adult stature approxi- mately at twenty-three to twenty-five, the latter at twenty to twenty-one years. The details of growth and the actual measurements vary considerably with race ; thus the supremacy of the American girl over her brother appears to be less marked and to cover a shorter period than that of the English, German, Swedish, or Italian girl. Children of well-to-do families are superior to 1 II. P. Bowditch : Eighth Annual Report of the State Board of Health of Massachusetts, 1877. 2 Roberts: Manual of Anthropometry, 1878. REPRODUCTION. 489 others in both weight and stature. Beyer1 has shown that systematic exercise may markedly increase both height and weight. Disease may alter the form of the curve of growth. But the final result seems to depend less upon external conditions than upon race and sex. As an interesting accessory fact it was found by Porter2 that well-developed children take a higher rank in school than less-developed children of the same age. Tf the percentage annual increase of the total weight he computed, it is found to diminish throughout life, very rapidly during the first two or three years, later more slowly and with minor variations of increase and decrease ; that is, as growth proceeds and the powers of the individual mature, the power to grow becomes rapidly less. This is a peculiar and most interesting fact, and has not been explained. It would seem to signify that the sum of the vital powers declines from birth onward. Many facts indicate that the common concep- tion, dating from the time of Aristotle, of human life as consisting of the three periods of rise, maturity, and decline, must give way to a more rational idea of a steady decline from birth. Puberty. — By puberty is meant the period of sexual maturity, at which the individual becomes able to reproduce. In the male the exact time of its onset, characterized primarily by the appearance of fully ripe spermatozoa, is not well known, but is believed to be about one year later than in the female. In temperate climates, therefore, it usually appears in boys not before the age of fifteen ; it is earlier in warmer regions. It is preceded and accompanied by acceleration in bodily growth, already spoken of. Other bodily changes, such as general maturation of the functions of the reproductive organs, alterations in the bodily proportions, increase of strength, and growth of the beard, all of which are elements of the transformation from boyhood to manhood, either occur at that time or follow soon after. One of the most obvious external change- i- that of the voice. Its tone may fall permanently an octave, and for the time being become rough, broken, and uncontrollable. This is due to a rapid general enlargement of the laryngeal cartilages and a lengthening of the vocal cords. In the girl the oncoming of puberty is marked more exactly than in the boy by the appearance of menstruation, in the majority of girls in temperate climates at the a«;e of fourteen to seventeen, lint other characteristic anatoin- ical and physiological changes in the body occur. The uterus, the external reproductive organs, and the breasts become larger, while the pelvis widen-. The prepubertal acceleration of growth has been mentioned. Nervous disor- ders are especially prone to make their appearance at this time. The subcuta- neous layer of adipose tissue develops and confers upon the outlines the grace- ful curves characteristic of the woman's body. The mental faculties mature, and the girl becomes a woman earlier and more rapidly than the boy a man. 1 II. G. Beyer: "The Influence of Kxercise on Growth," Journal of Experimental Medicine, 189(i, i. p. 546. See also "The Growth of U.S. Naval Cadets," Proceedings of the United States Naval Institute, 1895, xxi. p. 297. 2 W. T. Porter: "The Physical Basis of Precocity and Dulness," Transactions of the Acad- emy of Science of St. Louis, 1893, vi., No. 7. See also "The Growth of St Louis Children," Ibid., 1894, vi. No. 12. 490 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. Climacteric. — At the sixtieth year the power of producing spermato- zoa, and, therefore, the reproductive power of man, begins to wane. It con- tinues, however, in a diminishing degree, even to extreme old age, and there is no recognized period of ending of the male sexual life. In woman, on the other hand, the sexual period continues for only thirty to thirty-five years, and the climacteric, menopause, or change of life, marks a definite ending of the power of reproduction. In temperate climates it occurs usually between the ages of forty-four and forty-seven ; in warmer regions it comes early, in colder late. It is earlier in the laboring classes, and later where menstruation has first appeared early. Its most characteristic feature is the cessation of menstruation, which is a gradual process extending over a period of two or three years and characterized by irregularity in the oncoming and the quantity of the flow, and by gradual diminution. But the cessation of the menses is but one phenomenon in a long series of changes that pro- foundly affect the whole organism and endanger life. The reproductive organs and the breasts diminish in size, and ovulation ceases. The changes in the pelvic organs are in general the reverse of those occurring at puberty. The organic functions generally are rendered irregular; dyspepsia, cardiac palpi- tation, sweating, and vasomotor changes arc frequent; vertigo, neuralgia, rheumatism, and gout are not rare; a tendency to obesity occurs, though sometimes the reverse ; irritability, fear, hysteria, ami melancholia may be present ; the disposition may be temporarily altered ; all of which changes indicate that the female organism at this time sutlers a profound nervous shock. The loss of the weighty function of reproduction and the adaptation to the new order of events are not accomplished quietly. Senescence. — The progressive diminution in the power of growth from birth onward throughout life has been mentioned, and may be interpreted as indicating that the process of senescence begins with the beginning of life.1 In the broadest sense this is true, and is confirmed by a study of various organic functions. In the more restricted sense senescence or old age com- prises the period from about fifty years (in woman from the climacteric) onward, during which there is a noticeable progressive waning of the vital [lowers. The leading somatic changes accompanying old age are atrophic and degenerative, but detailed statistics of this period are almost wholly wanting. A marked cellular difference between the young and the old, which is shown by nearly if not quite all tissues, is the relatively large nucleus and small quantity of cytoplasm in the young, the proportions being reversed in the old. This has been pointed out as follows by Hodge" in the nerve-cells of the first cervical spinal ganglion : Volume of nucleus. Fetus (at birth) 100 per cent. Old man (at ninety-two years) 64.2 " 1 <'/. ( '. s. Minut : Journal of Physiology, 1891, xii. 2 C. F. Hodge: Anatomischer Anzeiger, 1894, ix.: Journal of Physiology, 1894, xvii. Nucleoli observ- Pigment Pigment able in nuclei. much. little. in •"))') per cent. in 5 " 67 per cent. 33 per cent. REPR 01) UCTION. 491 Thus with the progress of age the nuclei become small and irregular in out- line, and the cytoplasm pigmented, while the nucleoli are often wanting. The nuclear differences are even more marked in the cerebral ganglia of lues, where moreover, aged individuals possess a smaller number of nerve-cells than the young. The nuclear differences accord with the common belief thai the nucleus is the formative centre of the cell. It has been shown that a decrease in the weight of the whole brain occurs in both men and women, beginning in the former at about fifty-five years, in the latter at about forty-live years. In eminent men the decrease begins later. The thickness of the cortex and the number of tangential fibres in it diminish especially after fifty years, and this probably signifies a loss of cells. There is a decrease in general brain-power, in power of origination, in the power to map out new paths of conduction and association in the central nervous system and thus to form habits. Reaction- time is lengthened. The delicacy of the sense-organs is noticeably less, and in the eye the hardening of the crystalline lens and the weakening of the ciliary muscle diminish the power of accommodation. The muscles atrophy and mus- cular strength is reduced. The pineal gland, ligaments, tendons, cartilage, and the walls of the arteries, show a tendency toward calcification, and the bones become more brittle. Subcutaneous adipose tissue disappears, but a fatty de- generation of cells is not uncommon, notably in all varieties of muscle-cells, in nerve-cells, and probably in gland-cells. The pigment of the hairs disap- pears. The size of the muscles, the liver, the spleen, the lymphatic and prob- ably the digestive glands, decreases. The heart and the kidneys seem to retain their adult size. The vital capacity of the lungs, the amounts of carbonic acid and of urine excreted, diminish. The rate of respiration and of the heart-beat rises slightly. Ovulation is wanting, and the power of producing spermatozoa is lessened. The stature undergoes a slight and steady decrease. Boas ' has shown that in the North American Indian this continues from about thirty years of age onward. All of these changes, the details of which should be care- fully studied and reduced to anatomical and physiological exactness, demonstrate that senescence is characterized by a steady diminution of vitality. Death. — Sooner or later vitality must cease and the change thai is called death must come. The term "death" is used in two senses, according as it is applied to the whole organism or to the individual tissues "I' which the organ- ism is composed. The former is distinguished as somatic death, or death simply, the latter as the death of the tissues. Somatic death occurs when one or more of the organic functions is so dis- turbed that the harmonious exercise of all the functions becomes impossible. Thus, if the brain receives a ^vxvv<- concussion, the co-ordination of the organs may be interrupted ; if the respiration ceases, the necessary oxygen is withheld ; if the heart fails, the distribution of oxygen and food and the collection of wastes come to an end; if the kidney- are diseased, the poisonous urea is retained within the tissues. A continuation of any one of these profound abnormal conditions, which may be brought about by accident or disease, or a 1 F. Boas: Verhandlungen da- Berliner Anthropologischen QezeUschaft, L895. 492 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. simultaneous occurrence of several slight disturbances of function, such as is not infrequent in aged persons, may prevent the restoration of that concordance among the organs without which the individual cannot live. The most con- venient and most certain sign by which somatic death maybe recognized is the absence of the beat of the heart, and in nearly all cases this is the criterion employed. But it should be borne in mind that the failure of the heart to beat is but one of the causes, and frequently a very secondary one, the primary cause being then associated with other functions. It is at present in most cases quite impossible to trace the course of events by which the derangement of one function leads to the ultimate cessation of individual life. Death of the tissues or of the living substance is neither necessarily nor usually simultaneous with somatic death. Constantly throughout life the mole- cules of living matter are being disintegrated, and whole cells die and are cast away ; life and death are concomitants. With the cessation of the individual life the nervous system dies almost immediately. With the muscular tissue it is very different. The stopping of the beat of the heart is a gradual process, and, as Harvey long ago pointed out, the last portion to beat, the ultimum moriens, is the right auricle. For many minutes after death the heart, if exposed, will be found to be excitable and to respond by single contractions to single stimuli. Irritability is said to continue in the smooth muscle of the stomach and the intestines for forty-five minutes, and considerably later than this the striated muscles of the limbs can still be made to twitch by proper stimuli, in the cat and rabbit after twelve or fourteen hours.1 Gland-cells die probably within a few minutes. As to the chemical changes undergone by the protoplasm in the process of living, little can be said. The composi- tion of dead protoplasm is comparatively well known, that of living proto- plasm is at present largely a blank ; and, although investigation has gone suf- ficiently far to offer a basis for several suggestive hypotheses, the latter are too abstruse for lucid discussion in the present space. Neither in somatic death nor in the death of the tissues does the body lose weight. Within fifteen or twenty hours it cools to the temperature of the surrounding medium. Rigor mortis, due to the coagulation of the muscle-plasma within the muscle-cells, begins within a time varying with the cause of death from a half hour to twenty or thirty hours, and continues upon an average twenty-four to thirty- six hours. Then the tissues soften, and soon putrefactive changes begin. Theory of Death. — It has been intimated that all the tissues are destined to die. An exception must be made in the case of those germ-cells, both male and female, that are employed in the production of new individuals. They pass from one individual, the parent, to another, the offspring, and thus cannot be said to undergo death. This is the basis of Weismann's theory of the origin and significance of death in the organic world." According to Weis- mann, primitive protoplasm was not endowed with the property of death. As found in the simplest individuals, like the Anxrha, even at the present ' Lee, Adler, and Balkley : American Journal of Physiology, 1900, iii. p. xxix. 2 A. Weismann: Essays upon Heredity, 1889, i. RE PR OB UCTION. 493 day, with a continuance of the proper nutritive conditions protoplasm does not grow old and die; the single individual divides into two and life continues unceasing, unless accident or other untoward event interferes. With the progress of evolution, however, the cells of the individual body have become differentiated into germ-cells and somatic cells, the former subserving the reproduction of the species, the latter all tl ther bodily functions. Germ- cells are passed on from parent to offspring; they never die, they arc immor- tal. Somatic cells, on the other hand, grow old, and at last perish. Death was, therefore, in the beginning, not a necessary adjunct to life ; it is not inhe- rent in primitive protoplasm, but has been acquired along with the differen- tiation of protoplasm into germ-plasm and somatoplasm, and the introduction of a sexual method of reproduction. It has been acquired because it is to the advantage of the species to possess it; in the simplest cases it should occur at the close of the reproductive period, and in fact it frequently does occur then. A superabundance of aged individuals, after they have ceased to be reproduc- tive, would be detrimental to the race ; it is to the advantage of the species that they be put out of the way. Death of the individual in order that the species may survive has, therefore, become an established principle of nature. But the higher animals are better able to protect themselves from destruction than the lower, and, moreover, they are needed to rear the young ; hence in them the duration of life is frequently prolonged beyond the reproductive period. Weismann's theory has been the cause of much discussion, and the pros and cons have been set forth by eminent biological authorities. In its appli- cation to the human race it would seem that the factors of social evolution have brought it about that the aged are protected in the struggle for existence for long after their reproductive usefulness has ceased, and thus the working of a pitiless biological law has become modified. F. Heredity. Biologists are accustomed to recognize two factors as responsible for the character and actions of the living organism. These are heredity and the environment. Heredity includes whatever is transmitted, either as actual or as potential characteristics, by parents to offspring. The environment com- prises both material and immaterial components, such as food, water, air, or other substances that surround the organism, and the forces of nature, such as light, heat, electricity, and gravity, that act as conditions of existence or as stimuli to action. The same principles apply to the character and actions of every cell of a many-celled organism, but here we must include in the envi- ronmental factor the mysterious influences that are exerted upon the cell by the other cells of the body. Of these two factors heredity acts from within, the environment from without the living substance. Among unicellular or- ganisms the individual begins his career when the bit of protoplasm thai con- stitutes his body is separated from the parent bit of protoplasm. Among higher forms, including man, the term individual may be applied to the fer- tilized ovum; the union of the ovum and the spermatozoon inaugurates the 494 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. new being. From the inception to the death of the individual, life consists partly of manifestations of the powers conferred by the germ-cells and partly of reactions to environmental influences. In considering the details of vital action we are apt to overlook these fundamental tacts and to evolve narrow and erroneous views as to the causes of vital phenomena. Biologists are -ccking with increasing vigor to determine the relative importance of the parts played by these two principles in development and in daily life. It is need- le-- to say that the problem is a difficult one and is still far from solution. In previous chapters of this book attention has been directed more especially to the external than to the hereditary factor. A work upon physiology would be incomplete, however, if it did not include an examination of the latter, especially since at the present time heredity is one of the leading subjects of biological research and discussion. It is proposed, therefore, in this section to present a brief outline of the facts, the principles, and the attempted ex- planations of the modes of working of heredity. It should be premised that, because of the present incomplete state of our knowledge of the facts, the highly speculative and involved character of most of the theories, and the con- Stant, active shifting of ideas and points of view, such an outline must neces- sarily be incomplete and in many respects unsatisfactory. Pacts of Inheritance. — It is not proposed in this paragraph to enter into a discussion of the question as to whether a particular vital phenomenon is a fact of inheritance or a reaction to external influences. For our present pur- poses it is sufficient to record the common facts of resemblance to ancestors, and to assume that such resemblance, when present, has been inherited. Resemblances are strongest between child and parents, and appear in a dimin- ishing ratio backward along the ancestral line. Galton l has computed that, of the total heritage of the child, each of the two parents contributes one- fourth, each of the four grandparents one-sixteenth, and the remaining one- fourth is handed down by more remote ancestors. The correctness of this estimate has been disputed by Weismann. The fact must not be overlooked that, in addition to and back of all the particular individual features that are inherited, a host of racial characteristics are transmitted — the progeny of a given species belongs to that species; the human being is the father of the human child, the child of Caucasian parents is a Caucasian, of negro parents a negro. Congenital resemblances may be anatomical, physiological, or psychological, and in each of these classes they may be normal or pathological. Anatomical resemblances are the most commonly recognized of all : facial features, stature, color of eves and of hair, supernumerary digits, excessive hairiness of body, cleft palate, monstrosities, and various defects of the eye, such as those that give rise to hypermetropia, myopia, cataract, color-blindness, and strabismus, are all known examples. Physiological peculiarities that may be transmitted include the tendency to characteristic gestures, locomotion and other muscular movements, longevity or short life, tendency to thinness or obesity, handwriting, 1 Francis Galton : Natural Inheritance, 1889, p. 134. REPRODUCTION. 495 voice, haeniatophilia or tendency to profuse hemorrhage from .slight wounds, gout, epilepsy, and asthma. Psychological inheritances comprise habits of mind, talent, artistic and moral qualities, tastes, traits of character, tempera- ment, ambition, insanity and other mental diseases, and tendencies to crime and to suicide. Latent Characters ; Reversion. — Characters that never appear in the parent may yet be transmitted through him from grandparent to child ; such charac- ters are called latent. Among the most striking latent characters are those con- nected with sex. Darwin 1 says : " In every female all the secondary male characters, and in every male all the secondary female characters, apparently exist in a latent state, ready to be evolved under certain conditions." Thus, a girl may inherit female secondary sexual peculiarities of her paternal grand- mother that are latent in her father, or a boy may inherit from his maternal grandfather characteristics that never show in his mother. An excellent example of such transmission, taken from the hcrbivora, is the common one of a bull conveying to his female descendants the good milking qualities of his female ancestors. In the human species hydrocele, necessarily a disease of the male, has been known to be inherited from the maternal grandfather, and hence must have been latent in the mother's organism. That in such cases the character is really potential, though latent in the intermediate ancestor, is rendered probable by such well-known facts as the appearance of female cha- racteristics in castrated males, and of male characteristics in females with dis- eased ovaries or after the end of the normal sexual life. Latency may be offered as the explanation of the numerous cases of atavism, or reversion, by which is meant the appearance in an individual of peculiarities that were formerly known only in the grandparents or more remote ancestors, but not in the parents of the individual. This subject is one of the most important in the whole field of heredity. Almost any character may reappear even after many generations. In the human species stronger likeness to grandparents than to parents is a frequent occurrence. The majority of the frequent anomalies of the dissecting-room are regarded as reversions toward the simian ancestors of the human race. The crossing of two strains develops a strong tendency to reversion, and because of this the prin- ciple of atavism must constantly be taken into account by breeders of animals and growers of plants. As an example of reversion after crossing may be mentioned the well-known one, studied by Darwin, of the frequent appear- ance of marked stripes upon the legs of the mule, the mule being a hybrid from the horse and the ass, both of which are comparatively unstriped but are undoubtedly descended from a striped zebra-like ancestor. Here the capacity of developing stripes is regarded as latent in both the horse and the ass, but as made evident in the mule by the mysterious influence of crossing, Darwin thinks likewise that the customary degraded state of half-castes mav be due to reversion to a primitive savage condition which, usually latent in 1 Charles Darwin : The Variation of Animals parturition. Obviously, no one denies this possibility in the unicellular organisms, where reproduction by fission prevails, for there the protoplasm of the body of one parent becomes the substance of two offspring; in the transformation nothing is lost, and hence whatever peculiarities the ances- tral protoplasm has acquired are transferred bodily to the descendants. But in multicellular forms, where sexual reproduction exists, the case is very dif- ferent, for here whatever i- transmitted is transmitted through germinal cells, or germ-plasm, a- the hereditary substance contained in the germ-cells is now commonly called. The problem then resolves itself into that of the relation of tin' germ-plasm to the protoplasm of the rest of the body, the so-called somatoplasm; and the question to he answered is this: Are variations in the REPRODUCTION. 497 parental somatoplasm capable of inducing such changes in the germ-plasm thai somatic peculiarities appear in the offspring similar to those possessed by the parent? Weismann classifies all somatic variations according to their origin into three groups — viz. injuries, functional variations, and variations, mainly climatic, that depend upon the environment. The problem of their inherit- ance is a far-reaching one, and upon its correct solution depend principles that are of much wider application than simply to matters of heredity ; for if acquired characters can be inherited, there is revealed to us a most potent fac- tor in the transformation of species, and the whole question of the possibility of use and disuse as factors of evolution is presented. The larger evolutionary problem need not here be considered. Regarding the problem of the inheritance of acquired characteristics we may say at once that it is not yet solved. To the lay mind this may seem strange, for at first thought it appears self-evident that parents may transmit to their children peculiarities that they themselves have acquired. Affirmative evidence seems all about us, as witness the undoubted cases of inheritance of artistic tastes, of talent, of traits valuable in professional life, which seem to originate in the industry of the parent. But scientific analysis by Weismann and others of popular impressions, popular anecdotes and hearsay evidence, and accurate original observation, have revealed little that cannot as well be explained on other hypotheses. Anatomical and functional peculiarities of the body that are apparently new often reappear in successive generations, but to assume that they are acquired by the somatoplasm and have become congenital, rather than that they are germinal from the first, is unwarranted. Direct experiments by various investigators are almost as inconclusive. Weismann ' has removed the tails of white mice for five successive generations, and yet of 901 young every individual was born with a tail normal in length and in other respects. Bos2 has experimented similarly upon rats for ten generations without observing any diminution of the tails. The practice of circumcision for centuries has resulted in no reduction of the prepuce. The binding of the feet of Chinese girls has not resulted in any congenital malformation of the Chinese foot. Brown- Sequard,3 and later Obersteiner,4 have artificially produced epilepsy in guinea- pigs by various operations upon the central nervous system and the peripheral nerves, and the offspring of such parents have been epileptic. At first this would seem to amount to proof of the actual hereditary transmission <»(* mutila- tions, yet in these eases the mutilation itself was not transmitted ; the offspring were weak and sickly and exhibited a variety of abnormal nervous and nutri- tional symptoms, among which was a tendency Inward epileptiform convulsions, the cause of which is still to be explained. Evidence from palaeontology regarding the apparent gradual accumulation of the effects of use ami disuse throughout a long-continued animal series seems to require the assumption of 1 A. Weismann : Essays upon Heredity, vol. L, L889, p. 432. * J. R. Bos: Biologisches Centmlblatt, xi., 1891, S. 7:11. 3 E. Brown S('i|ii:u(l : Researches on Epilepsy, ''<•.. Boston, 1857; also various later papers. 4 H. Obersteiner : Medizmische Jahrbiicher, Wien L875, S. 17'.'. Vol. IT.— 32 498 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. such a principle as the inheritance of acquired characters, but even here the principle of natural selection may perhaps be equally explanatory. The Inheritance of Diseases. — The question of the inheritance of diseases has also been much discussed. The same general principles apply here as in the inheritance of normal characteristics. The fact has been mentioned above that pathological characters, whether anatomical, physiological, or psycholog- ical, are capable of transmission. If, however, a pathological character has been acquired by the parent and is not inherent in his own germ-cells, it is extremely doubtful whether it can be passed on to the child. A diseased parent, on the other hand, may produce offspring that are constitutionally weak or that are even predisposed toward the parental disease, and such off- spring may develop the parent's ailment. In such cases constitutional weakness or predisposition, and not actual disease, is inherited ; the disease itself later attacks the weak or predisposed body. Proneness to mildness or severity of, and immunity toward, certain diseases seem to be transmissible. These sub- jects, however, are so little understood, and the real meaning of such terms as predisposition, inherited constitutional weakness, and inherited immunity, is so little known, that it is idle to discuss them here. Considerable experimental work has been performed recently upon the transmissibility of infectious diseases. Undoubtedly infectious diseases cling to a particular family for generations. The transmitted factor is probably fre- quently, if not usually, simple predisposition. But in an increasing number of cases there appears to be transmission of a specific micro-organism. Such transmission is called germinal when the micro-organism is conveyed in the ovum or the semen, and 'placental or intra-uterine when the micro-organism reaches the fetus a Tier uterine development has begun, and chiefly through the circulation. Of germinal infection- syphilis seems undoubtedly capable of transmission within either the ovum or the semen. The possibility of germinal transmission of tuberculosis has been maintained, but is not fully proven. Of intra-uterine infections there have been observed in human beings apparently undoubted cases of typhoid fever, relapsing fever, scarlatina, endocarditis, small-pox, measles, croupous pneumonia, anthrax, syphilis, and possibly tuberculosis and Asiatic cholera. It is obvious that neither germinal nor placental inheritance, both taking place through the medium of a specific micro- organism, and not through the modification of germ-plasm, is comparable to inheritance in the customary sense. Theories of Inheritance. — From early historical times theories of inher- itance have not been wanting. Physical and metaphysical, materialistic and spiritualistic theories have had their day. Previous to the discovery of the spermatozoon (Hamm, Leeuwenhoek, 1677) all theories were necessarily fantastic, and for nearly two hundred years later they were crude. The theories that are now rife may be .-aid to date from 1864, when Herbert Spencer published his Principles <>f Biology. Since that date they have become numerous. Even the modern theories are highly speculative ; none can be regarded as being accepted to the exclusion of all others by a large REPR OD UCTION. 499 majority of scientific workers, and the excuse for introducing them into a text-book of physiology is the hope that a brief discussion of them may prove suggestive, stimulating, and productive of investigation. Germ-plasm. — Germinal substance, germ-plasm (Weismann), or, as it is some- times called, idioplasm (Nageli), must lie at the basis of all scientific theories of heredity. The father and the mother contribute to the child the sperma- tozoon and the ovum respectively, and within these two bits of protoplasm there must be contained potentially the qualities of the two parents. There is the strongest evidence in favor of the prevailing view that the nucleus alone of each germ-cell is essentially hereditary, or, more exactly, that the chromatic substance of the nucleus is the sole actual germinal substance. We have seen that the tail of the spermatozoon is a locomotive organ, and that the body of the ovum is nutritive matter. We have seen also that the essence of the whole process of fertilization is a fusion of the male and the female nuclei, or, more exactly, a mingling of male and female chromosomes. Hence most physiologists agree with Strasburger and Hertwig that the chromatic substance of the nuclei of the germ-cells transmits the hereditary qualities. As to the origin of the germ-plasm, two hypotheses have been suggested. Spencer, Darwin, Galton, and Brooks have argued in favor of a production of germ-plasm within each individual by a collocation within the reproductive organs of minute elementary vital particles — "physiological units" (Spencer), "gemmules" (Darwin) — which come from all parts of the body ; hence each part of the body has its representative within every germ-cell. This hypothesis affords a ready explanation of numerous facts, but its highly speculative cha- racter, the entire absence of direct observational or experimental proof of its truth, and the demand that its conception makes upon human credulity, mili- tate against its general acceptance. Weismann, the promulgator of the second hypothesis, denies altogether the formation of the germ-plasm from the body- tissues of the individual, and maintains its sole origin from the germ-plasm of the parent of the individual. Through the parent it comes from the grand- parent, thence from the great-grand pa rent, and so may be traced backward through families and tribes and races to its origin in simple unicellular organisms. According to Weismann, therefore, germ-plasm is very ancient and is directly continuous from one individual to another; the parts of an individual body are derivatives of it, but they do not return to it their repre- sentatives in the form of minute particles. The general truth of Weismann's conception can hardly be denied. As to the morphological nature of germ-plasm, two views likewise are held. One school, led by His and Weismann, holds that germ-plasm possesses a complicated architecture; that the fertilized ovum contains within its structure the rudiments or primary constituents of the various cells, tissues, and organs of which the body is destined to be composed ; and that growth is a develop- ment of these already existing germs and largely independent of surrounding influences. In accordance with this idea, segmentation of the ovum is specifi- cally a qualitative process, one blastomere representing one portion of the 500 AN AMERICAN TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY. future adult, another blastomere another portion, and so on. This theory recalls in a refined form the crude theory of Preformation that was advocated during the seventeenth and eighteenth centuries by Haller, Bonnet, and many others, according to which the germ-cell was believed to contain a minute but perfectly formed model of the adult, which needed only to be enlarged and unfolded in growth. The other modern school, in which Oscar Hertwig is prominent, maintains that the fertilized eg7 Alcohol, action of, on conductivity of nerves. 93 effect of, on nerve-currents, 156 fumes, action of, on nerves, 60 st imulating acl ion of, 7.") Alkalies, action of, On nerves and muscles, till Allantoic arteries, 17 1 vein, 474 Allantois, 171 Allochiria, 400 Ammonia, action of, on nerves, 60 Ammonium salts, action of, on muscles, 138 Amnion, 472 Amniotic cavity, 472 Amceba, contractility of, 19 Amoeboid movement, 19 in neuroblasts, 176 in ova, 22 Amphiaster, 169 Amphimixis as cause of congenital variation. 504 Amphioxus, reflexes in, 212 Amplitude of sound-waves, 381 Ampulla of 1 lenle, 1 17 Ampullae of the semicircular canals, 372 Ampullary nerves, stimulation of, 107 Amputation in man, effects of, on neurones, 196 Anaemia of the brain during fatigue, 288 Anaesthesia, contralateral, after hemisection of the cord, 233 Anaesthetics, action of. on nerve-currents, 155 Analgesia, 232 following removal of the cerebellum, 272 Analysis of composite tones, 364 Anatomy of the ear, 362 Anelectrotonus, 62 Angular movements of joints, lit! Anisotropic substance of muscle-fibres, 104 A nodal cont racl ion, .';•> Anode, physical, definition of, 52 physiological, definition of, 52 Anosmia, 411 Anterior association centre, 257 roots, recurrent sensibility of, 204 Aphasia, 257 Apraxia, 259 Aqueous humor, index of refract ion of. 303 Arteries, calcificati f. in old age, 191 Arthropods, segmental nervous system of, 212 \ it icular cart ilages, 415 Articulation. i:;i Articulations, varieties of, 414 Artificial circulation through the heart. 69 I In ough t he muscles. 68 stimulation of muscle compared with normal, 134 Aryepiglottic fold, I ! ! Aryteno-epiglottidean muscle. 126 Arytenoid cartilages, 125 muscle. 426 Asexual reproduction. 139 I heory of t lie origin of, 1 1 1 Aspirates. |.;7 Association centre, anterior, 257 middle, 257 posterior, 257 cerebral, varial ions in. 260 fibres and centres of the cortex. 256 tracts, origin Of, from central cells. 205 Astasia after removal of the cerehel In in. 273 Asthenia from removal of the cerebellum, 27:'. \-i igmat ic accommodal ion, •"•i|» Astigmal ism. ::i7 detect ion of, 319 irregular, 319 Astral rays, contractility of, 470 \tavisin. !!»."> 507 01 IS INDEX TO VOLUME II. Ataxia after removal of the cerebellum, 273 Atonia after removal of the cerebellum, 273 Atrophy of the oerve-cellB from disuse, 195 Atropia, action of, on accommodation, 311 Atropin, action of, '•'• ossicles. 365 movements of, 3<>7 sensations, limits of, 382 successive contrast in, 3S.s theory of, 380 Auritde of the external ear, 362 Automatism, definition of, 20 Axones, definition of, 21, 173 growth in diameter of, 179 length of, 174 Ball-and-socket joint, 416 Barium salts, action of, on muscles, 138 Bartholin i, glands of, 162 Basilar membrane, structure of, '■'•77 width of, 380 Bathyasthesia, 233 Beats in musical tones, production of, 386 Benham's spectrum top, oil Binocular combination of colors, 358 vision, 356 illusions in. .T>!i rivalry of the fields of vision in, 358 Biophors of the germ-plasm, 503 Birds, removal of cerebral hemispheres in, ~<>7 Birth, size of the child at, 487 Birth-rate of the two sexes, 483 Births, multiple, 182 ratio of male to female. 483 Blastomeres, 17" Blind spot, 328 Blood, amount of, in the central nervous system, 288 changes in, during pregnancy, 477 Blood-supply of the central nervous system. 286 relation of, to irritability, (17 Body-sense area of the cortex, 252, 254 Body-temperature, rise of, from injury to the optic thalami, 271 from lesions in the corpora striata, 271 Bolometer. 1 12 Bones, action of muscles on, 417 Bottcher's crystals, 1 15 Brain, curve of growth of, 279 growth of, 278 number of nerve-elements during, 2*0 relation of, to growth of the body, 280 size of neurone- during, 281 metabolic activity in, 288 regulation of circulation in. '.'-7 weight of, '-'7o Brain-stem, .'71 Brain-ventricles, capacity of, '-71 Brain-weight, decrease of, in old age, 296 relation of, to insanity, 278 to sex, 276 to social environment, 277 Brain-weights of different races, 278 Breaking contraction, point of origin of, '■>'< " I'.m aking " -hock. :;i Broca'e convolution, 257 Brown-Seqnard's paralysis. 233 Brucin, action of, <>n end-plates. -.'7 Bulbo-cavernosus muscle, 449 act ion of, in erection, 4til Bulimia, 104 Caffkin. action of. on coagulation of muscle- plasma, 164 Calcium salts, action of, on the muscles. L38 relation of, to irritability, 59 Canalis cochlearis, 373 reuniens, 373, 374 Capillary electrometer, 14b' ( 'aput gallinagims, v\ I Carbon dioxide, action of, on conductivity in nerves. 'X', on the nerves, 60 on warm spots, 398 effect of, on nerve-currents, 156 of t he muscle, 168 production of, in nerves, 95 disnlphide, action of, on nerves, 60 t lardiac palpitation at the climacteric, 490 Carnic acid of muscles, 167 ( larnin of muscles 167 I a-t ration, effects of, 463 on the voice. 431 Cataleptic rigor, 160 Caudate nucleus, heat centre of, 271 Cell, galvanic, 29 Cell -differentiation, 22 Cells, growth of, 486 Central cells, importance of. in relation to in- crease of organization. 285 nervous system, amount of blood in. 288 arrangement of cell groups in. 205 bh.od-supply of. 2N> change in specific gravity of, with age, 284 condition of, in sleep, 293 conscious phenomena of, 172 daily rhyi Inn- of, 289 development of. 172 fatigue Of, 289 general arrangement of, 202 functions of, 171 influence of the thyroid on growth of, 289 in old age, 29o intensity of metabolism in, 288 mednllation of nerves in, 181 operations on, in frogs, 265 organization of, at different ages, 284 neurones of the spinal cord, 203 stimulation of the nervous system, 208 Centre of hearing, cortical, 253 of rotation of the eye, 298 of smell, cortical, 253 of vision, cortical, 253 spinal, of ejaculation, 465 of erect ion, 4■'<■'> sensations in indirect vision, '■)'■'<'■'< phenomena of, 333 theories, 335 triangle, 334 vision, theories of, 335 Color blindness, 338 hereditary transmission of, l* 1 1 of the rods, 342 Colored shadows from simultaneous contrast, 347 Color-mixture, '■'.'■','■'. Colors, binocular combination of, 358 complementary, •'!■'> I physical basis of, 332 relative luminosity of, 340 saturation of, 342 i lombinational tones, 387 Commissure. Meynert's, 238 von Gidden's, 238 Commissures, origin of, from central cells. 205 Common sensation, definition of, 399 sensibility, afferent paths of the nerves of, 230 Commutators, method of using, 36 Complementary colors. :;:;i Composite tones, analysis of, 384 Conceptions, multiple, 182 Concha of the external ear, 362 Conduction by contiguity, 81 directions of, >1 from neurone to neurone, 84 in branching nerves, 80 in ganglion-cells, 97 in muscles, 80 in nerves, effects of, 95 in nerve-trunks, 79 of nerve-impulses, direction of, 184 from neurone to neurone. '.' of muscle, 17 of muscles, adaptation of, to their normal functions, 108 of ova, 22 of the astral rays, 17u Contraction curve of muscle, effect of frequeul excitat ion on, 115 idio-muscular, 92 of muscles, post-mortem, 160 relation of, to structure, 107 remainder, L06 wave in muscle, rate of transmission, >7 length of, B8 Contractions from repeated single stimuli, 112 introductory, 1 13 isomet ric, 110 isotonic, 1 lo normal, tetanic nature of, 132 of rigor calorie, 165 Contracture after frequenl excitation. 128 after single excital ion, 129 ilelinit ion of, 1 16 from fat [gue, 130 510 INDEX TO VOLUME II. Contracture in dying muscles, 132 in rigor mortis, L28 in veratria poisoning, 128 normal, 129 of the nick muscles after cerebellar injury, 272 pathological, 127, L32 relation of, to tetanus, 117. 122, 124 ( 'out ractures, 127 Contrast, visual, 3 16 in space perception, 352 Convergence of the eyes in accommodation, 311 muscular mechanism of, 300 Co-ordination of the efferenl impulses in re- flexes, -.'l 1 < lopulal ion, i<>3 ( tore-conductors, 158 Cornea, curvature of, 303 Cornicnlum laryngis, 125 Cornutine, action of, on muscles, 137 Corona radiata, 454 of the ovum, 150 Corpora cavernosa of the penis, 148 striata, functions of, •-'71 Corpus callosum, functions of, 27<» luteum, 455 spongiosum of the penis, 448 Corresponding points of the retinas, 359 Cortex cerebri, effects of localized electrical stimulation of, 241 electrical stimulation of. 242 number of nerve-cells in, 284 course of efferenl impulses from, 251 latent areas of, 261 Corti, cells of, 377 organ of, structure of, 377 rorls of, 377 Cortical areas, 213 motor, in man, 250 size of, '.'17 centres, 243 motor control, crossed, 251 multiple character of, 250 regions. 243 stimulation, inhibitory effects of, 224 Cowper's gland, 443 histology of, 448 secret ion of, 1 16 Crabs, regeneration of lost parts in, 4!»6 Cranial nerves, afferent, 236 Creatin in muscle, 166 ( Ireal inin of muscle, 167 Cretinism, sporadic, 289 t i ic arytenoid muscle, lateral, 426 posterior, 126 Cricoid cartilage, 425 Crico-thyroid muscles, 426 Criminals, weigh 1 of the brain in, 277 < 'rista acusl ica of the semicircular canals. 373 Critical period of nerve.-,, 66 Cross-suturing of nerve-trunks, 201 ( Iruciate heat cent re, 271 Cupola of the cochlea. 375 ( 'urare, act ion of, 26 Current - of acl ion in muscle, L50 in nerves, 153 of rest. 1 17 theories as to their cause, 148 Curve of fatigue with repeated single contrac- tions, 113 of intensity of sleep, 294 of musch contraction, effect of frequent exci- tation on, 1 15 of mnscular contractions, l1"1 of work for muscles, 1 l" Cutaneous sensations, cortical area for, 253 disturbance of, in disease, 103 Cutaneous sensations, varieties of, 390 temperature points, 398 Cytoplasmic changes in nerve-cells, 182 Daniell cell, 28 Darwin's tin cry of heredity, 501 Death, definition of, 491 of the tissues, 192 somatic, 191 theory of, 192 Decidua graviditatis, 461,471 menstrualis, 458, 461 reflex a, 172 serotina, 172 vera, 172 I decidual cells, 171 Defecation, cerebral control of, 270 reflex character of, 213 Degeneration after hemisection of the spinal co nl, 228 following removal of motor cortical areas, 244 of cut nerves, 78 of muscle after section of its nerve, 70 of nerve-cells, 199 of nerve-elements. 197 of nerves after section, 69 reaction of, 17, 54, 7(1 Deglutition, action of the epiglottis in, 422 Deiters' cells of the organ of Corti, 377 nucleus, 238 Demarcation currents of injured muscle, 148 Dendrites, definition of, 174 Dermal sensations, cortical area for. 253 path of conduction for, in the cord, 235 sensibility, area of distribution of the nerves of. 231 Descending impulses, course of, 244 Desiccation of nerve, 59 Determinants of the germ-plasm, 503 Deutoplasm of the ovary, 450 ovarian, composition of, 451 Development of nerve-cells, 176 Dextrose, action of, in delaying rigor mortis, 164 Diapedesis of maternal leucocytes into the fetus, 176 DiaTthrosis, 115 I >ia SOniC nerve-cells, 178 Differential tones, 3-7 Diffusion of central nerve-impulses, 208 of impulses in the cord, influences affecting, 217 of nerve-impulses, peripheral, 21s I ligasl lie muscle. 126 Digitalin, action of, on coagulation of niuscle- plasma, 164 on muscles, 137 Digitalis, action of, on nerve-, and muscles, 60 Digits, supernumerary, 194 Diopl lie apparatus of the eye, defects of, 314 Dioptric- of the eye, 300 1 >iopt i y. definition of, 30 1 Diphasic current of action, 152 Direction, judgments of, by means of auditory sensal ions, 389 of the nerve-impulse, 184 Discord, 3-7 Discriminating sensibility of the skin for pres- sure, 392 Discriminative sensibility for differences of tem- perature, 397 Discus proligerus, 450, 454 Diseases, inheritance of, 498 Dispermy, 171 Dispersion of light. 316 ; Dissociation of the axial and focal adjustments of the eye, 312 INDEX TO VOLUME II. 511 Distance, judgments of, by means of auditory sensations, 389 perception of, 354 visual perception of, 348 Disuse, effect of, on muscles, 77 Dizziness, 405 Dogs, removal of cerebrum in, 267 Domestication, effect of, on menstruation in animals, 460, 462 Dorsal nerve-roots, efferent fibres in, 203 roots, degeneration resulting from injury to, 227 spinal nerve-roots, number of fibres of, 230 Dreams, 293 Du Bois-Reymond's key, 30 law of stimulation, 32 theory of currents of rest, 148 Ductus cochlearis, structure of, 374 endolympliaticus, 373 venosus of the embryo, 476 Duration of electric currents, effect of, on their irritating power, 46 Dynamic equilibrium, organs of, 407 Dyspepsia accompanying the climacteric, 490 Dyspnoea, effect of, on the iris, 324 Ear, analysis of composite tones by, 384 anatomy of, 362 discriminative sensibility of, for pitch, 385 fatigue of, 387 imperfections of, 388 membranous labyrinth of, 372 ossicles of, 365 sensibility of, in perception of time intervals, 388 Earth-worms, regeneration of lost parts in, 496 Efferent fibres of the optic nerves, 240 impulses in reflexes, co-ordination of, 214 neurones of the dorsal spinal nerve-roots, 203 of the spinal cord, 203 paths from the cortex, course of, 244 Ejaculation, 465 Ejaculatory duct, 447 Elasticity of muscle, 105 Electric currents, correlation of their duration witli histological structures, 47 effect of, on muscles, 61 on nerves, 62 the duration of, 46 their density, 41 galvanic effect of, on normal human nerves, 51 influence of their direction in nerves, 48 of varying duration of, 47 methods of detecting, 145 spread of, in moist conductors, 41 stimulating effect of, 28 organ, 145 Electrical changes iii I lie retina, 331 phenomena of muscle and aerve, 144 of nerves, interpretation of, 158 st imulation of nerve, 25 of nerves, law of, :;■_' Electrodes, shielded, 1 1 varieties of, 29 Electrostatic changes, stimulating action of, 42 Electrotonic changes of conductivity, 50 of irritability, til in human nerves, 65 twitch, 157 Electrotonus, 62 Embryo, nutrition of, 175 rate of growth of. 1-7 Emmctropia, 313 Emmetropic eye. 312 Encephala, classification of, according to weigbt, 275 Encephalon, specific gravity of, 275 weight of, -.'7 1. ■-,7."i End-bulbs, sensory, 392 Endolymph, 372 End-organs, importance of, in touch sensations, 396 transmission of excitation by means of, 82 Energy liberated in contracting muscles, 138 Engelmann's theory of the nature of muscular contraction, 105 Environment, influence of, on organisms, 493 Epididymis, 1 17 Epigenesis, theory of, 500, 504 Epiglottis, 121 Epinephrin, action of, on muscles, 138 Equilibrium of the body, definition of, 404 relation of the cerebellum to, 273 sense of, 404 Erection, 464 spinal centre for, 464 Erector clitoridis muscle, 464 penis, action of, in erection, 464 muscle, 449 Eserin, action of, on nerve and muscle, 60 Ether, action of, on coagulation of muscle- plasma, 164 on conductivity of nerves, 93 effect of, on nerve-currents, 155 vapor, action of, on nerves, 60 Eustachian tube, 363 function of, 369 Excitability, changes in, during Wallerian de- generation. <>!> Exercise, effect of, on growth, 489 on muscular endurance, 76 Exhaustion of muscles, 72 Explosive consonants, 437 Extensibility of muscle, 105 External auditory meatus, 362 ear, anatomy of, 362 rectus muscle, 299 Extractives, nitrogenous, of muscle, 166 Extrapolar region, 62 Eye, abnormal positions of, after cerebellar injury, 272 adaptation of, to light, 326 axes of rotation of, 299 chromatic aberration of, 316 constants, changes in, during accommodation, 311 defects in the dioptric apparatus of, 314 dioptric apparatus of, 300 mechanical movements of, 298 movements, binocular co-ordination in, 300 extent of. 298 muscles of. 299 innervation of, 300 optical constants of, 303 power ill', ::n| positions of, 299 refractive media of, 302 surfaces of, 303 spherical aherral ion of, 315 V M.i.nri w tubes, 1 13, 156 False amnion, 173 Falsetto register of the voice, 133 Far-point of vision, 312 Fatigue, cerebral amemia from, 288 curve with repealed single contractions. 111! effect of. mi In i<_;lit of coui racl ion. 113 on muscular conl raction, 130 on rigor caloris, 165 morlis. Kin from voluntary muscular contraction, 134 in nerve-fibres, 195 of central nervous system, 289 512 INDEX TO VOLUME II. Fatigue of motor end-organs, 83 of muscle, 7 relation of, to muscular work, 74 Female births, relative number of, 483 pronucleus, 453 Females, rate of growth in, 488 Fenestra ovalis, '■'<*>'■'> rotunda, 363, 375 Use Of, 37b' Ferment, myosinogen-coagulating, L61 Fertilization, Ititi Fetal membranes, 472 Field of vision, binocular rivalry of, 358 Fimbriae of the Fallopian tube, 156 Fish, bony, removal of cerebral hemisphere in, 263 Fishes, semicircular canals in, 407 visual accommodation in, 306 Flicker photometry, 345 Focal illumination of the eye, 320 Foci, conjugate, 302 principal, 302 Foramen ovale of the foetal heart, 476 Forced movements after section of the semi- circular canals, 405 in frogs, 266 Fovea centralis, 327 Franklin's theory of color vision, 337 Frietionals, 137 Frogs, removal of cerebral hemisphere in, 264 striped muscle, time of single contraction in, 108 Frontal lobes of the hemispheres, effecl <>f re- removal of, 262 Fuhlsphare, cortical, 252 Fundamental tone, definition of, 383 Galvani, Luigi, 28 Galvanic current, action of, on conductivity, it) contracture effecl of, on muscles. 131 effect of, on muscles, 61 on nerves. 62 of making and breaking, 31 on normal human nerves, 51 opening and closing contractions with, 38 Galvanometers, 145 Galvaiiotonus, 54, 131 Gamogenesis, 1 1<» ( langliou spirale of the ear, 376 Ganglion-cells, conduction in, 97 Gaseous exchanges in the brain, 288 ( ia-.es of muscle, 168 Geminal fibres of the pyramidal tracts. 245 < temmules of t he germ-plasma, 499 < tenio-hyoid muscle, 426 Germinal spot of the ovary, 450 transmission of infectious diseases. Ill* vesicle, si ructnre of, 150 ( term -plasm as a basis of heredity, 499 continuity of, 502 detinit iOD Of, l!"l morphological nature of, 199 origin of, 199 I testation, duration of, 478 Gland-cells, electric currents in, 145 Glands of Bartholini, 162 of Fittre, 1 18 Glans penis, 1 1!' Gliding movements in joints, 41t; Glossopharyngeal nerve, gustatory function of, 410 Glossopharyngeus, central conducting paths of, 236 Glottis. 423 oedema of, 122 respiratory movements of, 429 ( HyCOCOl] in muscles, ll>7 < Hycogen of muscles, l<>7 Golgi, organ of, in tendons, 402 Graafian follicle, 15 1 Graphic method of studying muscular contrac- tions, 99 I ira\ ity, influence of, on cerebral circulation, 287 (irav matter of the cerebrum, water contents of, 274 Growth after birth. 187 before birth. 486 increase of fibres of the cortex during, 282 of functional neurones during, 282 influence of sex on the rate of, 488 influences which modify, 489 of nerve-cells, I7 Bering theory of color vision, 336 Hermann's theory of currents of rest. 1 I- Ileteromita. reproduction in, 440 Hinge-joints, 116 Histology of striped muscle, 104 Hofacker Sadler law. 1-1 Holmgren method for testing color vision, 339 Horopter, 359 Human muscles, fatigue of, with artificial stim- ulation, 134 INDEX TO VOLUME II 513 Hunger, 404 Hunger-centre, clinical evidence for, 404 Hydra, regeneration of lost parts in, 496 Hydrocyanic acid, action of, on coagulation of muscle-plasma, Ki4 Hymen. 462 Hyo-glossus muscle, 426 Hyperesthesia, homolateral, after hemisection of the cord, •!'■'•'■', Hypermetropia, 313 range of accommodation in, 314 Hypoxanthin of muscles, 167 Idants of the germ-plasm, 503 Idiomuscular contraction, 27, !'•_>, 128 Idioplasm as a basis of heredity, 499 Ids of the germ-plasm, 503 Illusions, visual, in sizes of objects, 354 of space perception, 351 Immunity, inherited, 498 Impregnation, 466 Incus, 366 Independent irritability of muscle, 25 Index of refraction of the aqueous humor, 303 of the lens, 303 of the vitreous humor, 303 Indifferent point of polarized nerves, 64 Indirect vision, color sensations in, 333 Induced currents, making and breaking shocks with, 40 prevention of spread of, 44 electric currents, stimulating effect of, 33 Induction apparatus, schema of, 33 Infections, intra-uterine, 498 Infectious diseases, germinal transmission of, 498 Inferior oblique muscle, 299 rectus muscle, 299 Inharmonic overtones, 386 Inheritance, facts of, 494 of acquired characters, 496 of diseases, 498 theories of, 498 Inhibition from cortical stimulation, 224 in the central nervous system, 224 Inorganic salts, relation of, to irritability, 59 Insanity, relation of brain-weight to, 278 variations of muscular tonus in, 220 Insect muscle, time of contraction in, 108 Intensity of visual sensations, 339 Intermedins nerve of Wrisberg, central path of, 236 Internal capsule, grouping of fibres in, 248 ear, anatomy of, 371 rectus muscle, 299 Intracranial pressure, relation of, to blood- pressure, 287 Intraocular images, 320 Intrapolar region, 62 Introductory contractions of a contraction series, 113 peak of tetanus curves, 124 Inversion of retinal images, 305 Invertebrates, conduction in the nerves of, 91 Involuntary muscles, rigor mortis of, 162 Ion-proteid compounds of muscle, 168 Iris, dilator nerves of, 32 t direct response to lifjlit by, •">'-' I innervation of, 323 movements of, in accommodation, 309 rate of, 325 muscles of, 323 relation of, to spherical alienation. 315 Irradiation in the retina, ''. 19 of nerve-impulses in the central nervous sys- tem, 208 Irritability, definition of, 20, 23 33 Irritability, effect of blood-supply on, 66 of constant currents on. i>2 of repeated stimuli on, 65 of muscle, 25 of nerve-fibres, 21 of nerves. 24 and muscles, conditions affecting, 55 effect of section on, 69 of ova, 22 Irritants, classification of, 23 conditions determining their efficiency, 28 effect of, on irritability, 55 of variations in strength of, 39 relation of, to the response, :.' 1 Ischio-cavernosi muscles, 449 Ischio-cavernosus, action of, in erection, 464 Isolated conduction in nerve-trunks, 79 Isometric contractions, definition of, 110 Isotonic contractions, definition of, 110 Isotropic substance of muscle-fibres, 104 Joints, classification of, 415 Jumping, 420 Kakyokinetic figures in mature nerve-cells, 21 12 Katelectrotonus, 62 Kathodal contraction, 115 Kathode, physical, definition of, 52 physiological, definition of, •">■.! Keys, electric, 30 Knee-kick, reinforcement id', 222 Krause's membrane, 104 Labia majora, 462 minora, 462 Labial consonants, 137 Labio-dental frietionals, 438 Labium tympanicum of the internal ear, 377 vest ibul are of the limbus, 377 Labor, nature of, 4*1 stages of, 179 Labor-pains, 479 Labyrinth of the ear, anatomy of, 371 Lactation, ovulation (luring, 15(1 Lactic acid of muscles, L68 Lamina spiralis, 372 Lamina- of medullary tube in the foetus, 205 Laryngeal muscles, specific actions of, 428 nerve, recurrent, 428 superior. 128 Laryngoscope, 129 Larynx, earl ilages of, 425 closure of, during muscular effort, 423 muscles of, 125 nerves of, 128 -,t met ore id', 421 Latent areas of the cortex, 261 characters, hereditary transmission of. 195 period, effed of temperature on. 136 of tension on. l lit of inoi in- end plates, 103 of muscle, 103 of red muscle. L09 of re1 inal stimulation, 343 of simple muscular cont raction, in-.' " Law of contraction,*' Pfluger'8, 50 Law of stimulation of human nerves by bat ter\ currents. 54 Lecit hin of nerves, L6fl Lemniscus, medial. 226 sensory paths entering, 235 Lens, changes in. during accommodation, 307 crystalline, changes in. -with old age, 31 I curvatures of, 303 opacities in. 321 refractive index of. 303 5] 1 INDEX TO VOLUME 11. Lens, thickness of, 303 Lenticular ganglion, 311 Leucin, action of, on end-plates, 27 Leucocytes, movements of, 19 Leucophrys patula. reproduction of, 142 Life of the individual, stages of, 4 — ■ * > Ligaments of tin- malleus, 366 of the incus, :!<>? Light, changes in the retina produced by, 330 definition of, 298 dispersion of, 316, :;:;•_' monochromatic, 316 physical theory of, :;.;i rays of the luininiferous ether, 331 sensations, intensity of, 332, 339, mechanism for the production of, 331 quality of, 332 Light-waves, lengths of, :;:;■.' Limbus of the spiral lamina, 37? Lingual frictionals, 138 uerve, gustatory function of, 410 Linguo-palatal consonants, 437 Liquids, 136 LiquoT amnii, 472 folliculi, 454 Listing's law, 299 Littre, glands of, 448 Load, effect of, mi the contraction curve, 111 of muscle, effect of. on latent period, 110 Local signs of sensations, 394 Localization, cutaneous, variations of, 395 in the skin, theory of, 395 of cell-groups in the cerebral cortex, 241 of cortical cell-groups for different impulses, 252 of pain sensations, 399 of touch sensat ions. III! I power, relation of, to mobility, 394 Locomotion. 120 Locomotor ataxy, disturbance of equilibrium in. 405 mechanisms, action of, 11 1 Long tracts of the cord, terminations of, 235 Long-reed register of the voice, 432 Loudness of the voice, factors determining, 430 physical cause of. :;~i Luminiferous ether, rates of vibration of, 331 Luminosity, relative, of spectral colors, 340 Luminous sensations, intensity of, 339 Lustre in visual sensations, explanation of, 3r>s MICROCEPHALIC brains, weight of. 275 Macula acustica, 3?:: lutea, 3-.'? .Macula- acusticse, relation of. to static equilib- rium. 407 Making contraction, point of origin, 35 " Making " shock. 31 Mai'- births, relative number of, 483 pronucleus. Pit; Mali-, rate of growth in, 1-* Malleus. 365 ligaments of. 366 Mammary glands. II.'!. 462 in pregnancy, 17? Manubrium of the malleus, 365 Masticatory movements, effect of, on taste sen- sations, 11 1 Mastoid antrum, 363 Maturation of germ-cells, significance of, 451 of nerve-cells. 177 of spermatozoa, 1 l"> of tin- ovum, 151 Meatus auditorius interims, 373 Mechanical stimulation of nerve. •.'.">, 56 strain, influence of, on neuroblasts, 1 Tf > work of muscular contract ion. 13* Medial lemniscus, 226 Medullary sheath, development of, in the cen- tral nervous system, 181 in the peripheral nerves, 180 significance of, 180 tube, fetal. 204 lamina- of, in the foetus, 205 Medullation, central, progressive character of, 181 of nerve-fibres, significance of, 283 peripheral. 180 Medusse, rate of conduction in, 89 staircase contractions in, 112 Membrana basilaris, 374 Saccida, 365 granulosa of the Graafian follicle, 454 reticulata, :;7- tectoria, 377. 379 tympani, 364 Membrai f Beissner, 374, 379 Membranous labyrinth of the ear, 372 Menopause, 459, 490 Meiisi mat ion, 457 age of onset of, 459 cessation of, at the climacteric, 490 general disturbances accompanying, 459 in animals. 460 relation of ovulation to, 456 theory of, 460 Mental activity, relation of cerebral circulation to, 288 Menthol, action of, on cold nerves, 398 Metabolism, intensity of, iii the brain, 288 Meynert's commissure, 238 Microcephalic brains, weight of, 275 Microcephalics, 268 Micturition, cerebral control of, 270 reflex character of, 213 Middle ear, 3112 inflammatory disease of, 364 mechanism of, 368 muscles of, 369 Migration of neuroblasts, 176 Modiolus, 372 Molecular weight, relation of, to physiological action, 60 Monochromatic light, 316 Mmis Veneris, 162 Monstrosities, congenital. 494 origin of, 483 Morgagni, ventricles of. 422 Morula, 470 Motor areas, cortical serial arrangement of, 247 degeneration after removal of. 244 paralysis following removal of. 269 physiological characters of. 213 subdivision of, into centres. 217 centres, degree of separateness of. 248 of the human cortex, 250 disturbance from bemisection of the cord, 230 end-plates, latent period of. 103 i ransmission of excitation by means of, 82 nerves, fatigue of, 96 rate of conduction in, 89 Movements of joints, varieties of, 416 of spermatozoa. 1 1 1 of the eyeball. 298 Multiple conceptions, 182 conl rol from t he cortex. 250 Muscse volitantes, 320 Muscle, accelerator urimi\ 449 aryteno-epiglottidean, 42 histological changes due to functional activity in, I!'-.' human, size of, 17 I internal si rucl lire of, 1 T! ' maturation of, 177 morphology of, 17:'. number of, in the central nervous system, 283 ii ii t ri 1 ion of, 190 nutritive com vol of, over nerve-fibres, ins Of animal-, size of, 175 of spinal ganglia, development of, 178 peculiarities of. 1 7 I pyramidal, 17* rate of discharge from. L89 regeneration of, 201 relation of si/.e and function in. 175 senescence of. 182, 490 significance of the branches of, 186 516 INDEX TO VOLUME II. Nerve-cells, summation of stimuli in, 190 volume relations of. 175 Nerve-elements, primitive segmental arrange- ment of, 205 Nerve-endings in tin- skin, 392 Nerve-fibres, classification of, '-'l cortical, increase in the aumber of, during -i-owili. 282 fat igue in, L95 functions of, 21 reaction of, 170 Nerve-impulse, definition of, 25 direction of tin- passage of. 1-1 electrical variation accompanying, 1-:; generation of, 1-7 in peripheral nerves. i-:; peripheral diffusion of. 218 reversed, in spinal ganglion-cells, L85 theories of, !'? transmission of, from neurone to neurone, 207 Nerve-muscle preparation, :;i Nerve-trunks, isolated conduction in, 7!) Nerves, action currents in, 153 auditory, central path of, 237 chemistry of, 169 cross-suturing of, 200 current of rest in, 1 1!' degeneration id', after section, til', 78 fatigue of, 75 glossopharyngeal, central conduction paths for, 236 in man, stimulation of, "1 law of stimulation of. with galvanic currenl . 50 limitation of the rate id' stimulation in, 126 lingual, gustatory function of, 410 medulla! ion of, 180 non-medullated, rate of conduction in, 90 of common sensation, central conduction paths of. 230 of cutaneous sensation, central conduction paths of, 233 of dermal sensation, area of distribution of, 231 of invertebrates, rate of conduction in, 91 of taste, nuclei of origin of, 236 of temperature, 397 of Wrisberg intermedins), central path of, 236 olfactory, central paths of, 241 optic, central paths of. 238 rate of conduction in, 89 secondary degeneration of, 197 specific energy of, 232 trigeminal, central paths of, 238 vagus, course of the afferent Mores in, 236 Nervi erigentes, 164 Neuroblast, development of. IT*; Neuro-kerat in of nerves, 169 Neuromuscular spindle. 390 Neurone, definition of, 173 Neurones, 21 afferent, to the spinal cord, 203 changes in number and size of, 280 conduction in. !'7 connection between, 206 double conduction in. 185 increase in number of, during growth, 282 internal structure of. 179 polarity of. 1-1 total number of. 283 Nicotin, action of, on end-plates, 27 on sympathetic ganglia, 219 Nissl method for study id' nerve-cells, 195 substance, iron in. 191 of nerve-cell-, 170 Nitrogen, free, of muscles, 168 Nitrogenous extractives of muscle, 166 Nodal point in the simplest dioptric system, 301 Nceud vital. 236 Noises, definition of, 388 Non-medullated nerves, rate of conduction in, 90 stimulation fatigue of, L80 Non-polarizable electrodes, 29 Nose, anatomy of. 108 respiratory t ract of, 408 Nutrition of nerve-cell.-. L90 of the embryo, 475 Nutritive control of mrve-cell bodies over nerve-fibres, 198 Nymphse, 462 Nystagmus after cerebellar injury, 27'.' < it l LOMOTOB nerve, ciliary fibres of, 311 relation of, to the iris, 323 Odors. 110 (Edema of the .-lot t is. 422 < ild age of the central nervous system, 295 Olfactory area id' the cortex, 253 cells, 108 epithelium. 10- nerves, central paths of, 241 paths to the brain, 409 stimuli, conditions affecting, 109 tracts, section id', in sharks, 264 ( hno-hyoid muscle. 425 Ontogenetic development of nerve-cells, 177 Onychodromus, reproduction of, 442 Oocyte, 4r.l < Ophthalmometer, 304 Ophthalmoscope, 326 Optic commissure, decussation of optic fibres in, 238 nerve, currents of action in, 154 nerve-fibres, number of, 330 nerves, central paths of, 238 cortical centres of, 240 efferent fibres of, 240 thalami, functions of, 271 Optical constants of the eye, 303 illusions in binocular vision. 359 of space, perceptions, :J>51 properties of striated muscle, 103 < Optograms, 330 Organ of Corti, structure of, :;77 of Golgi in tendons, 402 I Organization in the central nervous system, 285 relation of educahilitv to the establishment of, 286 Organs, growth of, 486 Oscillatory activity of the retina, 344 ( Os orbiculare of the incus, 366 I Ossicles, auditory, 365 of the ear. movements of, 367 Otitis media. 364 ( Otoconia, 37 l otolith-. :;7l Ova, 140 number of. in human ovary, 451 ( Ovaries, 1 13 effect of removal of. on menstruation, 459 st rucl me id'. 154 Overtone-, definit ion of. :!-:: inharmonic, 386 Oviduct-. 1 13, 150 Ovulation, 155 ( Ovum, chemist ry of, 450 fertilization of. 166 human, structure of. 1 I'' maturation of, 151 physiological properties of. 22 segmental ion of, 167 Stages in t he maturation of. 152 Oxygen, storage of, in muscle, L69 Supply, relation of, to irritability, lis IXDEX TO VOLUME II. 517 Pacinian body, 391 of the penis, 449 Pain nerves, evidence for the existence of, 232 points of the skin, 400 sensations of, 399 transferred or sympathetic, 400 Pale striped muscle, phvsiological peculiarities of, 109 Pangenesis, Darwin's theory of, 501 Papilla foliata of rabbits, 410 Paradoxical contraction. 157 Parallax, use of, in estimation of distance, 356 Paralysis after removal of motor areas, 269 agitans, 296 Brown-Sequard's, 233 homolateral, after hemisection of the cord, 233 Paramcecium, reproduction in, 440 Paramyosinogen, 163 temperature of heat coagulation of, 165 Paresis following removal of the cerebellum, 272 from injury to motor areas, 269 Partial tones, definition of, 383 Parturition, 479 spinal centre of, 481 Paths of conduction in the cord, clinical evi- dence on, 234 Pendular vibrations, 381 Penis, 443 structure of, 448 Perilymph, 372 Periodic reflexes, 216 Peripheral nerves, medullation of, 180 reference of special sensations, 400 Pfliiger's law of contraction, 50 Phakoscope, 308 Phalangar process of the rods of Corti, 378 Phosphenes, pressure, 305, 331 Photometry, 345 Phrenic nerve, currents of action in, 154 Phylogenetic development of nerve-cells, 177 Physiological anode, definition of, 52 kathode, definition of, 52 observations on afferent paths in the cord, 229 rheoscope, 148, 151 salt solution, 59 Physostigmin, action of, on accommodation, 311 on the eye, 325 Pia mater, weight of, 274 Pigment epithelium, retinal, movements of, 330 retinal, relation of, to adaptation of the eye, 326 Pince myographique, 87 Pineal gland, calcification of, in old age, 491 Pinna of the ear, 362 Pitch, limits of perception of, 382 of musical tours, 381 of the voice, 130, 432 Pituitary membrane, 408 Pivot-joint, 417 Placenta, IT I Placental transmission of infectious diseases, 498 villi, 17 1 Plants, regeneration of lost parts in. !!><; Pohl'a mercury commutator, '■>*> Polar amphiaster of the ripening egg, 153 bodies, 151 of the ovum, 453 Polarity of neurones, 184 Polarization, after-effects of, (;."> physiological, of neuroblasts, 1 T« ; Polarizing current, effect of. on conductivity, 50 on muscles, o'! on nerves. 62 Pole-changers, :: Positive after-images, 346 Posterior association-centre, 257 Post-ganglionic fibres of the sympathetic sys- tem, 219 Posture sense, 399 Potassium salts, action of, on muscles, 138 relation of, to irritability, 59 Preformation theory of heredity, 500 Pre-ganglionic fibres of the sympathetic system, •_'l!l Pregnancy, effects of, on the mother, 17? Presbyopia, 314 Pressure, effect of, on irritability of nerves, 56 infiuence of, on conductivity, 92 Pressure-points of the skin. '■'>'.)<> Pressure-sensations, fusion of, 394 Pressure-sense, delicacy of, 392 of the tympanic membrane, 382 Primary position of the eye, 299 taste-sensations, 412 Principal foci in a dioptric system, 302 point of the simplest dioptric system, 301 ray in the simplest dioptric -ysteru, 301 Processus brevis of the malleus, 365 gracilis sive folianus of the malleus, 365 Projection system of fibres, origin of, from cen- tral'cells, 205 Pronucleus, female, 453 male, 166 Proptosis after cerebellar injury. 272 Prostate glands, 1 13 histology of, 4 18 secret ion of. 1 Hi Prostatic lluid. 14(5. IIS Protagon of medullary substance. l?(i Proteids of muscle, precipitation temperature of, 166 of muscle-serum, 166 relation of, to muscular work, 74 Pseudoscopic vision, 318, 357 Psychical powers of the spinal cord, 215 Psycho-physic law, 340 of Fechner, 393 Puberty, 489 Pupil, changes during accommodation in. 311 in size of, 323 condition of, in sleep. 325 dilator nerves, 324 size of, in old age, 314 Pupillary reflex to light. 323 Purkinje-Sanson's images, 307 l'urkinje's figure, 321 phenomenon, .".In explanation of. :: 12 Purposeful reflexes, 215 Pyramidal fibres. Dumber of, 246 t racts in t lie cord, 2 15 geminal fibres of, 215 si/.e of. 252 nerve-cells, development of. it-' 3onance of the ear, 388 Resonants, 136 Resonators, analysis of sounds by, 385 Bete vasculosum of the testis, l'7 Betina, changes produced by light in, 330 circulation in. 322 histology of, 329 oscillatory activity of, '■'■ 1 1 Space percept ions by, 3 18 structure of, 327 Ret inal currents, 331 images, inversion of, 305 size of, 305 stimulation, after-effect of, 345 fatigue in. 34 l Ret inal stimulation, latent period of, 343 laws of, 343 rise to maximum for different colors, 343 vessels, demonstration of, 321 Ri version to ancestral characters, 495 Bheocord, 11 Bheonome, .'il Rheoscope, physiological, 148 Rbeoscopic frog, 1 18 Rheostat, 40 Bhinencephalon, 241 Rigor caloris, 57, 164 contracture in, 128 effect of fatigue ou, 165 mortis, 159 chemical changes accompanying, 162 contracture of, 128 disappearance of, 162 influence of the nervous system on, 220 nature of changes in, 161 Rima glottidis, 423 respiratoria, 423 vocalis, 423 Ritter's opening tetanus, 37, 61 tetanus, 131 Rod-and-cone layer, function of, 327 Rod-pigment, 339. See also Visual purple. Rods and cones, function of, 341 number of, 330 of Corti, 377 retinal, function of, 341 Rota i ion, movements of, 416 Running, 421 Rut of animals, 460 Sacculus of the internal ear, 373 Saccus endolymphaticus, 373 Saddle-joint, 41(i Salivary secretion, cerebral control of. 270 Salts, inorganic, relation of, to irritability, 58 of muscle, 168 of heavy metals, action of, on nerve and muscle, (ill Santorini, cartilage of, 122, 425 Sarcode of sponges, contractility of, 20 Sarcolactic acid, formation of, in rigor mortis, 161, 162 in clotting of muscle-plasma, 164 relation of, to fatigue contracture, 131 Sarcoplasm, 104 Saturation of colors, 342 Scala media, 375 tympani, 372, 37."> vest ibuli, 372, 375 Schneiderian membrane, 408 Scrotum, 443 Secondary degeneration of nerves, 197 position of the eye, 299 tetanus, 150 Secretion, salivary, cerebral control of, 270 Secretory nerves, fatigue of, 96 Segmental arrangement of nerve-elements. 206 react ions, reflex, 210 Segmental ion, h;7 Segmentation-centrosomes, 469 Segmentation-nucleus. 466 Semen, composition of, 445 Semicircular canals, membranous, 373 of the bony labyrinth, :'.?1 relation of, to equilibrium, 405 sect ion of, 105 Seminal vesicles. 1 13 function of, I 18 secretion of, I l'> Semivowels, 436 Senescence of nerve-cells, 182 of the central nervous system, 295 INDEX TO VOLUME II. 519 Senescence, phenomena of, 186 Sensation, cutaneous, definition of, 390 muscular, definition of, 390 of after-pressure, 394 of light, mechanism for the production of, 331 of temperature, 3!»7 Sense of equilibrium, 404 of touch, 392 Sensory areas of the cortex, determination of, 253 conducting paths in the spinal cord, 234 continuation of, in the brain, 235 cortical areas in man, 255 motor responses from, 253 relative functional importance of, 270 disturbance from hemisection of the cord, 230 impulses, path of, in the central nervous sys- tem, 226 relation of, to the maintenance of the erect posture, 419 nerve-endings in skeletal muscle, 402 in tendon, 402 in the skin, 391 nerves, rate of conduction in, 91 paths, degeneration of, after section of the dorsal roots, 227 in the central nervous system, 226 regions of the cortex cerebri, 252 stimulation, relation of, to sleep, 291 Sex, characters of, 442 of offspring, determination of, 483 origin of, 441 relation of brain-weight to, 276 Sexual characters, 442 glands, accessory, 445 organs, 443 reproduction, 440 congenital variations resulting from, 501 theory of origin of, 441 Shark, reflexes in, 212 removal of cerebral hemispheres iu, 263 Shrapn ell's membrane, 365 Siamese twins, 483 Simple muscular contraction, duration of, 102, 108 explanation of, 101 Simultaneous contrast, 347 Singing voice, 13 1 Size, increase of the embryo in, 487 of nerve-cells, 175 Skiascopy, detection of astigmatism by means of, 319 Skin, tactile areas of, 395 Sleep, 291 cause of, 292 condition of the pupils in, 325 curve of intensity of, 294 effects of loss of, 295 responsiveness to stimuli in, 293 Smell, 108 comparative physiology of, 409 subjective sensations of, 410 Smooth muscle, rate of conduction in, 89 Snails, regeneration of lost parts in. i!tt> Somatic death, 191 Somatoplasm, definition of, 496 Somatopleure, 172 Sound, physical, 381 Sounds, quality of, 383 Sound-waves, amplitude of, 381 composite, 384 limits of perception of, -'I-0-' production of, 381 Space illusions, 354 Space-perception from visual sensations, :;it Specific energies of nerves, doctrine of, 232, 399 Specific energy of the optic nerve. 331 gravity of muscle, 159 of the encephalon, 275 of the nervous system at different ages, 28 I nerve-energy, doctrine of, 232, 399 Spectral colors, incomplete saturation of, 342 Spectrum, 332 luminous intensity of the colors of, 340 top, Benham's, 31 1 Speech, dependence of, on hearing, 431 elements of, 133 Speech-centre. 257 Spermatids, 445 Spermatocytes, 445 Spermatozoa, 1 lo contractility of, 20 discovery of, 1 13, 498 entrance of, into the uterus, 465 locomotion of, 4 Tectorial membrane, 379 Tegmen of the tympanum, 364 Temperature, effect of. on muscular contraction, 136 influence of, on conductivity. 92 on irritability. 56 on rigor mortis, 161 limits ill' muscular contraction, 136 nerves, 397 of the blood from the brain, 288 rise of, from lesions of corpora striata. 271 sense, 397 spots of tin- skin. 39S Tendon rethxes after cerebellar injury, 272 sensory nerve-endings in, 402 Tension, effect of, on contraction curve, 109 on irritability of nerves and muscles, 56 on latent period. Ill) Tensor fcympani muscle, 369 Tentacles of Actinia-, contractility of, 20 Terminal arborizations, definition of, 174 Tertiary positions of the eve, 299 Testis, 443 ducts of, 1 17 histology of, 1 16 Tetanic contractions, height of, 120 relative intensity of, 126 Tetanomotor, 56 Tetanus. 66 anal} sis of, 123 complete, 120 curves, introductory peaks of. 124 explanation of, 121 from strychnin poisoning, '.'17 iiii iplete, 1 17 normal physiological, L32 of the muscles, 127 rati- of stimulation required for. 125 Bitter's, 37, 61. 131 Tetanus, secondary, 150 voluntary, 133 W'undt's' :;7 closing, 61 Thalamus, cortical connections of, 271 heat -cell tie of. 271 Thermal energy liberated in muscle. 141 stiiuulat ion of nerve, 25 Thermopile, 142 Thermotaxis, relation of cerebrum to, 270 Thirst. 10 1 Thyro-arytenoid muscles, 424, 426 Thyro-hyoid muscle, 425 Thyroid cartilage, 425 gland, relation of, to growth of the central nervous system, 289 Tigroid of nerve-cells, 179 Timbre of musical tones. 383, 387 Time intervals, perception of, by the ear, 388 Tissue death, 492 Tissues, growth of, 486 Tobacco smoke, action of, on nerves, 60 Tones, combinational. 387 differential, 387 fundamental, 383 loudness of, 381 pitch of, 381 simple. :\-\ Tongue, distribution of taste-sensations on, 413 Tonus, muscular, in the insane, 220 reflex origin of. 220 of muscles, 143 Touch illusions, 396 sensations, 392 localization of, 394 Tractus solitarius, 236 Tremors, 132 Trigeminal nerves, central paths of, 238 Triplets, 1-:; Trophic impulses to muscles, 70 influence of neurones on one another, 197 nerves of the muscles. 70 Tubuli recti of the testis. 1 17 Turtle's striped muscle, time of conti'action in, 108 Twins, 482 Tympanic membrane. 364 effect of destruction of, 370 pressure-sensations of, 382 vibrations of, 370 Tympanum, 363 mechanics of, 368 Ultimum moriens, 492 Umbilical arteries, 174 vein. 17 1 Umbo of the tympanic membrane, 365 Unconsciousness, 293 Unipolar excitation for localized excitation, 45 nerve-cells, development of, 178 stimulation, •';" principles of, 43 Urea in muscles, 167 Urethra, 143 structure of, 1 18 Uric acid iii muscles, 167 Uterus, 1 13, 156 Utriculus of the internal ear. 373 Vagina, 143, 162 Vagus, central path of the afferent fibres in, 236 nerve, fatigue of, 96 rate of conduction in. 90 Variation of the offspring in reproduction, 500 Variations, somatic, classification of, 497 Yas deferens. 1 17 INDEX TO VOLUME II 521 Vasa-deferentia, 443 efferentia of the testis, 447 Vaso-niotor nerves of the cranial vessels, 286 Ventral nerve-roots, number of fibres of, 230 Ventricles of Morgagni, 422 of the brain, capacity of, 274 Ventricular bands, 422 Veratria, action of, on coagulation of muscle- plasma, 164 on muscular contraction, 129, 137 on nerves and muscles, 60 effect of, on muscular contraction, 128 Vertigo in diseases of the ear labyrinth, 406 Vestibular root of the auditory nerve, central path of, 237 Vestibule of the bony labyrinth, 371 Vibrations of the tympanic membrane, 370 transmission of, through the labyrinth, 376 Vision, binocular, 356 far-point of, 312 indirect, 341 near-point of, 312 pseudoscopic, 357 stereoscopic, 357 Visual area of the cortex, 253 impulses, place of origin of, in the retina, 327 judgments of distance, 348 of size, 350 and distance, 354 purple, 330 adaptation of the eye by, 326 sensation, intensity of, 339 Vitelline membrane, absence of, in human ova, 450 Vitreous humor, opacities in, 321 refractive index of, 303 Vocal cords, false, 422 true, 423 Voice, 430 changes at puberty in, 489 effect of age on, 431 pitch of, 432 registers of, 432 Voice-production, 421 mechanism of, 431 Voices, classification of, 433 Volta, 28 Voltaic pile, 28 Voluntary muscular contractions, fatigue of, 134 tetanic character of, 133 reactions, afferent paths of, 226 anatomical mechanism of, 226 compared with reflex, 225 von Gudden's commissure, 238 Vorticella. movements of, 20 Vowel-sounds, 131 differences in quality of, 385 production of. 434 Vulva, 443, 462 Walking, 420 Wallerian degeneration, changes of excitability in, 69 of nerve-fibres, 197 of nerves, 69 Water, percentage of, in brain and cord, 274 pure, toxic action of, on nerves and muscles, 58 Weber's law, 340 applied to pressure-sensations, 393 Weight, increase of, in the embryo, 487 of the brain and spinal cord, 274 decrease of, in old age, 296 relation of, to social environment, 277 of the child at birth. 487 Weissmann's theory of heredity, 502 Whispering, 436 Whistling register of the voice, 433 White matter of the central nervous system, water contents of, 274 Womb, 443, 456 Work done by contracting muscles, condition^ affecting, L39 by muscular contraction, curve of, 140 Worms, segmental nervous system of, 212 Wrisberg, cartilages of, 422, 425 Wundt's closing tetanus, 37, 61, 131 Xantiiix of muscles, 167 Young-Helmholtz theory of color vision, :;:;" Zollner's lines. :;r.i Zona pellucida, 4~>4 of the ovum, 450 radiata of the ovum, 449, 450 GENERAL INDEX. Abdominal muscles, action of, in vomiting, i. 387 respiratory action of, i. 407 respiration, definition of, i. 398 Absolute muscular force, ii. Ill Absorbents, i. 318 Absorption, effect of alcohol on, i. 535 in the small intestine, i. 313 in the stomach, i. 312 mechanism of, i. .">12 nature of process, i. 27 of fats, i. 317 of gases by liquids, i. 414 of proteids, i. 316 of sugars, i. 317 of water and salts, i. 318 part played by leucocytes in, i. 48 paths of, i. 311 spectrum of oxyhemoglobin, i. 41 Accelerator centre, cardiac, i. 177 respiratory, i. 457 nerves of the heart, i. 167, 169 uriuse muscle, ii. 449 Accessory articles of the diet, i. 357 thyroids, i. 268 Accommodation, ii. 306 associated movements of, ii. 311 astigmatic, ii. 310 dissociation of, from convergence, ii. 312 influence of drugs on, ii. 311 in old age, ii. 314 mechanism of, ii. 309 nervous mechanism of, ii. 311 normal range of, ii. 312 range of, in hypermetropia, ii. 314 in myopia, ii. 314 relation of, to perception of distance, ii. 356 voluntary character of, ii. 311 Acetic acid, i. 536 Acetone, relation of, to fat metabolism, i. 539 Acetonitril, i. 5 12 Acetonuria, i. 537 Acetyl-acetic acid. i. 537 Acetyl-propionic acid, i. 538 Achromatic lenses, ii. 316 Achromatism of the eye, ii. 316 Achroodextrin, i. 285, 566 Acid, acetic, i. 536 acetyl-acetic, i. 537 acetyl-propionic, i. 538 amido-acetic, i. 5:;? amido-ethyl-sulphonic, i. 543 o-amido-a-thiopropionic, i. 546 aspartic, i. 557 benzoic, i- 569 butyric, i. 539 capric, i. 5 1 1 caproic, i. 540 caprylic, i. 511 carbamic, i. 5 is carbolic, i. 569 carbonic, chemical structure of, i. 545 choleic, i. 543 cholic, i. 543 Acid, chondroitic. i. 578 cynurenic, i. 571 diamido-acetic, i. 551 a-e-diamido-caproic, i. 552 diamido-valeric, i. 552 dithio-diamido-ethideue lactic, i. 547 fellic, i. 543 formic, i. 534 glutamic, i. 558 glycerin phosphoric, i. 559 glycuronic, i. 567 hippuric, i. 339, 569 homogentisic, i. 570 hydriodic, i. 509 hydrobromic, i. 509 hydrochloric, i. 507 hydrocumaric, i. 570 hydrofluoric, i. 510 iso-butyl amido-acetic, i. 540 iso-valerianic, i. 539 lactic, i. 545 levulic, i. 538 malic, i. 55s mercapturic, i. 547 metaphosphoric, i. 514 methyl amido-acetic, i. 538 monobasic fatty, i. 532 nucleic, i. 579 oleic, i. 560 orthophosphoric, i. 514 oxalic, i. 557 oxaluric, i. 555 oxybutyric, i. 548 oxyphenyl-acetic, i. 570 oxyphenyl-amido-propionic, i. 570 palmitic, i. 541 parabanic, i. 555 phenaceturic, i. 569 phenyl-acel ic, i. 569 propionic, L. 538 salts of muscle, ii. 168 sarco-lactic, i. 546 silicic, i. 519 stearic, i. 511 succinic, i. 557 sulphuric, i. 506 sulphurous, i. 506 thiolactic, i. 517 thymic, i. 57!' uric. i. 322, 338, 551, 557 Acidity of worked muscles, ii. 168 Acids, action of, on nerves and muscles, ii. 60 effect of, on pancreas, i. 236 AcinUS, delinit ion of, i. 212 Acquired characters, inheritance of, ii. 496 variat ions, ii. 500 Acromegaly, i. 278 Ad [nic rays of the luminiferous ether, ii. 331 Action current, diphasic, ii. 152 in t he heart, ii. 152 in the muscles, ii. 150 in t he nerves, ii. 15.",. IS.", \damkie\vic/. reaction for proteids. i. 576 Adam's apple, ii- 125 523 524 GENERAL INDEX. Addison's disease, i. 271 Adenin, i. o.i'.K 55 1 Adipocere, i. 5 1 1 . •">•>!• Adrenal bodies, internal secretion of, i. 272 removal of, i 271 secretory nerves of, i. 272 extract, action of, on muscles, ii. 138 physiological action of, i. 271 Aerial perspective, ii. 355 Afferent impulses, effect of, on irritability of the central nervous system, ii. "J23 neurones of the spinal cord, ii. 203 paths in the cord traced electrically, ii. 230 traced histologically, ii. 229 traced physiologically, ii. 229 respiratory nerves, i. 460 After-birth, 'ii. 481 After-effect of retinal stimulation, ii. 345 After-images, ii. 346 After-loading of muscles, ii. 110 After-pressure, sensation of, ii. 394 Agamogenesis, ii. 439 Age, changes in organization of the central ner- vous system with. ii. 284 influence of, on heat production, i. 482 on nerve-cells, ii. 490 on pulse-rate, i. 121 on respiration, i. 425 on visual accommodation, ii. 314 relation of body-temperature to, i. 469 of brain-weights to, ii. 276 of menstruation to, ii. 459 specific gravity of the nervous system with changes in, ii. 28 1 Air, alveolar, composition of, i. 413 atmospheric, composition of, i. 410, 413 complemental, i. 4:27 expired, composition of, i. 410 inspired, composition of, i. 410 in the Lungs, renewal of, i. 413 passages, obstruction of, i. 452 residual, i. 427 respiratory changes in, i. 410 stationary, i. 427 suction of, into veins, i. 97 supplemental, i. 427 tidal, volume of, i. 426 variations in the composition of, i. 435 Albinos, condition of the internal ear in, ii. 407 Albuminates, i. 577 Albuminoids, digestion of, in the stomach, i.297 enumeration of, i. 577 nutritive value of, i. 277, 349 properties of, i. 579 protection of proteids by, i. 349 tryptic digestion of. i. 304 Albuminous glands, i. 216 Albumins, properties of, i. 577 Albumose injections, effect of, on blood-coagu- lation, i. ii'.' Alcaptonuria, i. 570 Alcohol, absorption of. in the stomach, i. 313 action of. on conductivity of nerves, ii. 93 amyl, i. 539 cerotyl, i. 540 cetyl, i. 540 effect of, on nerve-currents, ii. 156 ethyl, i. 535 fumes, action of, on nerves, ii. 60 melicvl, i. 540 nutritive value of, i. 358 physiological action of, i :'..">7, 535 propyl, i- 536, 538 stimulating action of. ii. 7"> toxic effects of. i. 359 Alcoholic fermentation, i. 535 Alcohols, mon atomic, i. 531 Aldehydes, general properties of, i. 534 Aldoses, i. 561 Alimentary canal, movements of, i. 369 principles, i. 276 Alkalies, action of, oh nerves and muscles, ii.60 Allantoic arteries, ii. 474 vein. ii. 171 Allan toin, i. 555 Allautois, ii. 474 Allochiria, ii. 400 Alloxuric bases, i. 338, 339, 552 Altitude, effect of, on the number of red cor- puscles, i. 46 Alveolar air, compostion of, i. 413 capacity, i. 427 tension of carbon-dioxide, i. 413 of oxygen, i. 413 Alveolus, glandular, definition of, i. 212 Amido-acetic acid, i. 537 Amido-acids, properties of, i. 538 Amines, definition of, i. 541 Ammonia, action of, on nerves, ii. 60 inhalation of, i. 440 occurrence of, i. 511 origin of, in the body, i. 511 properties of, i. 511 Ammoniacal fermentation of urine, i. 512 Ammonium carbamate, i. 548 carbonate, i. 523 cyanate, i. 542 magnesium phosphate, i. 527 salts, action of, on muscles, ii. 138 Amnion, ii. 472 Amniotic cavity, ii. 472 fluid, inhibitory effect of, on respiration, i. 464 Amoeba, contractility of, ii. 19 Amoeboid movement, ii. Ill in neuroblasts, ii. 176 in ova, ii. 22 of leucocytes, i. 48 Amphiaster, ii. 469 Amphimixis as cause of congenital variation, ii. 504 Amphioxus, reflexes in, ii. 212 Ampho-peptone, definition of, i. 293 Amplitude of sound-waves, ii. 381 Ampulla of Henle, ii. 447 Ampulla? of the semicircular canals, ii. 372 Ampullary nerves, stimulation of, ii. 407 Amputation in man, effects of, on neurones, ii. 196 Amvgdalin, fermentative decomposition of, i. 542 Amyl alcohol, i. 539 Amylodextrin, i. 566 Amyloid, i. 578 Amylolytic enzyme of gastric juice in the dog, i. 296 of SUCCUS entericus, i. 308 of the liver, i. 330 enzymes, definition of, i. 280 action of. in the bodv, i. 285 Amylopsin, i. 232, 280 action of. on starch, i. 566 digestive action of, i. 305 occurrence of. i. 304 properties of, i. 305 Anabolism, definition of, i. 19 AnSBmia of the brain during fatigue, ii. 288 Anicsthesia, contralateral, after hemisection of the cord, ii. •.':;:; Anaesthetics, action of, on nerve-currents, ii, 155 effect of, on body-temperature, i. 472 Analgesia, ii. 232 following removal of the cerebellum, ii. 272 Analysis of composite tones, ii. 384 Anatomy of the ear, ii. 362 GENERAL INDEX. :>-i:> Aiielectrotonus, ii. 62 Angular movements of joints, ii. 416 Animal foods, composition of, i. 278 heat, i. 467 source of, i. 474 Anisotropic substance of muscle-fibres, ii. 104 Annulus Vieussens, i. 159 Anodal contraction, ii. '■',*'< Anode, physical, definition of, ii. 52 physiological, definition of, ii. 52 Anosmia, ii. 411 Antalbumid, i. 293 Anterior association centre, ii. 257 roots, recurrent sensibility of, ii. 204 Antilytic secretion, i. 230 Antimony poisoning, i. 514 Anti-peptone, definition of, i. 293 nature of, i. 302 An ti peristalsis, intestinal, i. 383 of the stomach, i. 379 Antrum pylori, i. 377 Apex beat, i. 117 preparation of the frog's heart, i. 188 ventricular, rhythmicity of, i. 151 Aphasia, ii. 257 Apncea, definition of, i. 440 foetal, i. 464 phenomena of, i. 44 relation of vagi to, i. 442 Apomorphia, action of, i. 389 Apraxia, ii. 259 Aqueous humor, index of refraction of, ii. 303 Arabinose, i. 562 Arginin, i. 552 Argon of the blood, i. 417 Aromatic compounds in urine, i. 572 metabolism of, i. 568, 569 Arsenic poisoning, i. 514 Arterial blood-pressure, explanation of, i. 92 pulse, cause of, i. 93 definition of, i. 139 extinction of, i. 94 Arteries, calcification of, in old age, ii. 491 coronary, i. 179 elongation of, i. 140 rate of flow in, i. 101 Arthropods, segmental nervous system of, ii. 212 Articular cartilages, ii. 415 Articulation, ii. 434 Articulations, varieties of, ii. 414 Artificial circulation through the heart, ii. 69 through the muscles, ii. 68 respiration, circulatory effects of, i. 453 methods of maintaining, i. I hi stimulation of muscle compared with normal, ii. 131 Aryepiglottic fold, ii. 422 Aryteno-epiglottidean muscle, ii. 426 Arytenoid cartilages, ii. 425 muscle, ii. 426 Asexual reproduction, ii. 439 theory of the origin of, ii. 441 Asparagin, i. 558 Aspartic acid, i. 557 Asphyxia, i. 411 circulatory changes in, i. 445 effects of, on the blood-vessels, i. 202 on the respiratory rhythm, i. 125 stages of, i. 145 Aspirates, ii. 437 Aspiration of the thorax, influence of, on the circulation, i. 77. 95 on the lymph-flow, i. 1 17 on venous circulation, i. 77. 95 Assimilation, general characteristics of, i. 19 Associated respiratory movements, i. 408 Association centre, anterior, ii. 257 Association centre, middle, ii. 257 posterior, ii. 257 cerebral, variations in, ii. 260 fibres and centres of the cortex, ii. 256 tracts, origin of, from central cells, ii. 205 Astasia alter removal of the cerebellum, ii. 273 Asthenia from removal of the cerebellum, ii. 273 Astigmatic accommodation, ii. 310 Astigmatism, ii. 317 detection of, ii. 319 irregular, ii. 319 Astral rays, contractility of, ii. 470 Asymmetrical carbon atom, definition of, i. 515 Atavism, ii. 195 Ataxia after removal of the cerebellum, ii. 273 Atelectasis, i. 396 Atmospheric air, composition of, i. 410, 413 Atonia after removal of the cerebellum, ii. 273 Atrophy of the heart after section of the vagi, i. Iti7 of the nerve-cells from disuse, ii. 195 Atropin, action of, on accommodation, ii. 311 on salivary glands, i. 222, 229 on sweat glands, i. 260 on the eye, ii. 325 effect of. on body-temperature, i. 472 Attraction sphere of the ovum, ii. 449 Auditory area of the cortex, ii. 253 canal, ii. 363 epithelium of the utricle and saccule, ii. 373 judgments, ii. 389 meatus, external, ii. 362 nerves, central paths of, ii. 237 cochlear division of, ii. 376 subdivision of, ii. 373 ossicles, ii. 365 movements of, ii. 367 sensations, limits of, ii. 382 successive contrast in, ii. 388 theory of, ii. 380 Augmentor centre of the heart, i. 177 nerves of the heart, i. 161, 167 Auricle of the external ear, ii. 362 systolic changes in, i. L15 Auricles, connection of. i. 135 degree of emptying, in systole, i. 138 functions of, i. L35 influence of, on venous blood-flow, i. 136 negative pressure in, i. 137, 138 Auricular pressure, i. 135, 137 systole, duration of, i. 124, 136 ' effect of, on venous blood-flow, i. 138 on ventricular tilling, i. 137 Auriculo-ventricular valves, i. 108 Auscultation, i. L18 Automal ism, deflnil ion of. ii. 20 Axilla, temperature in. i. 468 Axones, definition of. ii. 21. 173 growth in diameter of, ii. 179 length of, ii. 171 |: \. ii kiai. decomposition in the intestines, i. 309 Ball-and-sockel joint, ii. 1 16 Banl ing diet, i. 353 Barium salts, action of, on muscles, ii. 138 Barometric pressures, effecl of, on respiration, i. 134 Bartholin, duct of. i. 217 Lilands of. ii. 162 Basilar membrane, structure of. ii. 377 width of. ii. 380 Basophiles, i. 17 Baths, influence of, on body-temperature, i. 471 Bathyasthesia, ii. 233 Beats in musical tones, production of, ii. 386 Beckmann's apparatus, i. 68 526 GENERAL INDEX. Beef-tea, physiological action of. i. 359 Beer, i. 535 Beeswax, i. 540 Benham's spectrum top, ii. 344 Benzoic arid, i. 340, -— >• ii • Benzol, molecular constitution of, i. 56?! BenzopjTol, i. 571 Bidder's ganglion, i. 1 1- Bile, amount secreted, i. 246, 321 antiseptic property of, i. 326 composition of, i. 245, 32J discharge of, from the gall-liladder, i. 248, 249 fatty acids of, i. 541 influence of. on emulsification of fats, i. 307 mineral constituents of, i. 530 physiological value of, i. 325 pigments of, i. 245, 322 relation of, to fat absorption, i. 325 secret ion of, i. 246 sulphur of, i. 507 Bile-acids, i. 245 detection of, i. 324 Neukomm's test for, i. 545 occurrence of, i. 323 origin of, i. 324 Pettenkoi'cr- test for, i. 324, 544 relation of, to fat absorption, i. 326 Bile-capillaries, i. 244 Bile-ducts, occlusion of, i. 249 Bile-pigments, i. 322 chemical properties of, i. 574 Gmelin's test for, i. 322, 574 origin of, i. 45. 530 Bile-salts, i. 245 chemistry of, i. 543 circulation of, i. 544 Bile-secretion, normal mechanism of, i. 248 relation of, to blood-flow in the liver, i. 247 Bile-vessels, motor nerves of, i. 248 Biliary fistula, i. 321 Bilicyanin, i. 571 Bilirubin, i. 245, 574 Biliverdin, i. 245. 574 Bilixanthin, i. 571 Binocular combination of colors, ii. 358 vision, ii. 356 illusions in. ii. 359 rivalry of the fields of vision in. ii. 358 Biopbors of the germ-plasm, ii. 503 Birds, removal of cerebral hemispheres in, ii. 267 Birth, size of the child at. ii. 187 Birth-rate of the two sexes, ii. 483 Births, multiple, ii. 482 ratio of male to female, ii. 483 Biuret, i. 5 1!' Bladder, urinary, movements of, i. 369, 390 vaso-motor nerves of, i. 209 Blastomeres, ii. 17o Blind-spot. ii. 328 Blood, i. 33 amount of, in the central nervous system, ii. 288 changes in, during pregnancy, ii. 477 chemical composition of, i. 50 circulation of, i. 76 coagulation of, i. 51 defibrinated, i. 34 distribution of, in the body, i. 63 foreign, action of, on the heart, i. 192 ■ n- exchanges of, i . Ill general function of, i. •'!:; histological structure of, i. 33 identification of, i. 57:; oxidations in, i. 123 reaction of, i. 34, 290 regeneration of, after hemorrhage, i. 63 specific gravity of, i. ::i Blood, total quantity of, in the body, i. 63 transfusion of, i. 64 Blood-corpuscles, inorganic salts of, i. 50, 530 varieties of, i. 33 Blood-gases, analyses of, i. 411 extraction of, i. 420 tension of, i. 415 Blood-leucocytes, i. 17 Blood-plasma, color of, i. 33 composition of, i. 51 inorganic salts of, i. 50 Blood-plates, i. 49 Blood-pressure, aortic, i. 91 capillary, i. 84, 93 effect of the accelerator nerves on, i. 170 of the depressor nerves on. i. 173 on renal secretion, i. 253, 256 mean, definition of, i. 90 methods of measuring, i. 84, 85 origin of, i. 91,92 pulmonary, i. 91 respiratory changes in, i. 447 venous, i. 91, 94 Blood-serum, composition of, i. 51 definition of, i. 34 mineral constituents of, i. 530 osmotic pressure of, i. 68 Blood-supply of the central nervous svstem, ii. 286 relation of, to irritability, ii. 67 Bodily metabolism, estimation of, i. 343 movements, effect of, on lymph-flow, i. 147 temperature, effect of, on respiratory ex- changes, i. 132 Body-sense area of the cortex, ii. 252, 254 Body-temperature, rise of, from injury to the opt ic thalami. ii. 271 from lesions in the corpora striata, ii. 271 Body-weight, influence of, on heat-productiou, i. 482 loss of, from starvation, i. 362 Bolometer, ii. 142 Bones, action of muscles on, ii. 417 Border-cells of the gastric glands, i. 237, 238 Bottcher's crystals, ii. 445 Brain, circulation in. regulation of, ii. 287 curve of growth of, ii. 279 growth of, ii. 278 number of nerve-elements during, ii. 280 relation of, to growth of the body, ii. 280 size of neurones (luring, ii. 281 metabolic activity in, ii. 288 vaso-motor nerves of, i. 203 weight of, ii. 273 Brain-stem, ii. 274 Brain-ventricles, capacity of, ii. 274 Brain-weight, decrease of, in old age, ii. 296 relation of, to insanity, ii. 278 to sex. ii. 27 Brown-Sequard's paralysis, ii. 233 Brucin, action of, on end-plates, ii. 27 Brunner's glands, i. 243 Buffy coat, i. .">5 Bulbo-cavernosus muscle, ii. 149 action of, in erection, ii. 464 Bulimia, ii. 404 Butvric acid, i. 539 GENERAL INDEX. 527 Cadaverin, i. 543 Caffein, i. 553 action of, on body-temperature, i. 472 on coagulation of muscle-plasma, ii. 164 on kidneys, i. 254 Calcium, absortion of, i. 525 carbonates, i. 524 chloride, i. 523 excretion of, i. 526 fluoride, i. 510, 523 phosphates, i. 523 physiological value of, i. 524 relation of, to heart muscle, i. 151 salts, action of, on the heart, i. 190 on the muscles, ii. 138 amount of, in fibrin, i. 58 excretion of, i. 356 nutritive value of, i. 356 relation of, to blood-coagulation, i. 57, 524 to irritability, ii. 59 sulphate, i. 523 Calorie, definition of. i. 504 Calorimetric equivalent, i. 478 Calorimetrv, direct and indirect, i. 365, 475, 478 Canalis cochlearis, ii. 373 reuniens, ii. 373, 374 Cane sugar, injection of, i. 317 inversion of, i. 565 Capacity of the heart-ventricles, i. 105 Capillaries, biliary, i. 244 blood, length of, i. 79 permeability of, i. 70 pressure in, i. 84 rate of flow in, i. 101 resistance in, i. 81 structure of, i. 80 time spent by the blood in, i. 103 secretion, of the fuudic glands, i. 238 Capillary circulation, microscopic characters of, i. 80 electrometer, ii. 146 pressure, origin of, i. 93 relation of, to lymph-formation, i. 72, 75 Capric acid, i. 541 Caproic acid, i. 5 HI Caprylic acid, i. 541 Capsules, suprarenal, extirpation of, i. 271 Caput gallinaginis, ii. 464 Carbaniic acid, i. 548 relation of, to urea formation, i. 336 Carbamide, i. 548 Carbo-hsemoglobin, nature of, i. 39 Carbohydrates, absorption of, i. .':I7 affinity of cell-substance for, i. 568 chemistry of, i. 56] combustion equivalent of, i. 365 definition of, i. 561 digestion of, in the stomach, i. 296 dynamic value of, i. 175 fermentation of, in the intestines, i. 310 molecular constitution of, i. 561 nutritive value of, i. 277, 353 origin of fat from, i. 352 proteid-protection by, i. 56H synthesis of, i. •_'<> Carbon dioxide, action of, on conductivity in nerves, ii. '*'■> on the heart, i. I'll on the nerves, ii. fill on warm spots, ii. 398 dyspnoea, i. 444 etfeet of, on nerve-en rrents. ii. 156 elimination, conditions affecting, i. 429 cutaneous, i. 122 estimation of, i. 428 inhalation, effects of, i. 1 10 Carbou dioxide, occurrence of, i. 517 of muscle, ii. 168 of the blood, extraction of, i. 517 production of, in nerves, ii. 95 properties of, i. 518 tension of, in the alveoli, i. 413 in the blood, i. 416 disulpbide, anion of, on nerves, ii. 60 equilibrium, definition of, i. 345 metabolism of, i. 518 monoxide haemoglobin, i. 517 absorption spectrum of, i. 44 composition of, i. 38 inhalation, i. 440 properties of, i. 517 occurrence of, i. 516 properties of, i. 516 Carbonic acid, chemical constitution of, i. 545 Carburetted hydrogen inhalation, i. 440 Cardiac centre, augmentor, i. 177 inhibitory, i. 176 cycle, analysis of, i. 122 definition of, i. 101 duration of, i. 123 dyspnoea, i. 11 1 excitation, propagation of, during vagus stim- ulation, i. 163 impulse, i. 117 nerves, anatomy of, i. 159 classification of, i. 171 extrinsic, i. 159 of frogs, i. 160 of mammals, i. 160 palpitation at the climacteric, ii. 490 Cardio-inhibitory centre, respiratory variations in, i. 451 Cardio-pneumatic movements, i. 412 Cardiogram, i. 117 Cardiometer, i. 106 C'arnic acid of muscles, ii. 167 Carnin, i. 551 of muscles, ii. 167 Casein, i. -jtil composition of, i. 579 curdling of, by acids, i. 296 by rennin, i. 295 Castration, effects of, ii. 463 on t he voice, ii. 131 Cataleptic riijor, ii. 1(50 Catalysis, i. •>•_', 503 < Saudate nucleus, heat-centre of, ii. 271 Cell, galvanic, ii. 29 Cell-differentiation, i. 22; ii. 22. Cell-division, i. 20 Cell-granules of the glandular epithelium, i. 216 ('ells, growth of, ii. 186 Central cells, importance of, in relation to in- crease of organization, ii. 285 nervous system, amount of blood in, ii. \>,ss arrangement of cell groups in. ii. 205 blood-supply of, ii. 286 change in specific gravity of, with age, ii. 284 ( lit ion of, in sleep, ii. 293 conscious phenomena of, ii. 17'.' daily rhyt Inns of, ii. 289 development of, ii. L72 fatigue of, ii. 289 general arrangement of, ii. 202 fund ions of. ii. 171 influence of the thyroid <>n growth of, ii. 289 in old age. ii. 295 intensity of metabolism in, ii. 288 medullation of nerves in, ii. ls| operation on, in frogs, ii. 265 528 GENERAL INDEX. Central nervous system, organization of, at dif- ferent ages, ii. 28 l neurones of tin- spinal cord, ii. 203 stimulation "t' the oervous system, ii. 28 Centre, augmentor of the heart, i. 17? cardio-inhibitory, i. 176 defecation, i. 387 deglutition, i. :s?7 expiratory, i. 457 inspiratory, i. l.~>7 micturition, i. 391, 393 of bearing, cortical, ii. 253 of rotation of the eye, ii. 298 of smell, cortical, ii. 253 of vision, cortical, ii. 253 peripheral reflex, i. 178 respiratory, i. 455 salivary, secretory, i. 230 spinal, of ejaculation, ii. 465 of erection, ii. 46 1 of parturition, ii. 1-1 sweat, i. 260 thermogenic, i. 491 vaso-motor, i. 198 vomiting, i. 389 Centres, association, ii. 256 Centripetal nerves of the heart, i. 171 Centrosome, i. 22 of human spermatozoa, ii. Ill of the fertilized egg, ii. 168 of the ovum, ii. 449 f erebelluin. anatomical connections of, ii. 273 effects of injury to, ii. 272 functions of, ii. 272 senile changes in, ii. 296 Cerebral circulation, i. 203 conditions affecting, ii. 288 crossed, i. 443 cortex, relation of, to the vaso-niotor centre, i. 202 hemispheres, effect of removal of, ii. 263 relative physiological values of, ii. 259 removal of, in birds, ii. 266 in dogs. ii. 262 Cerebri n, i. .v> t\ 1 alcohol, i. 540 Characters, acquired, inheritance of, ii. 496 Chemical reagents, action of, on irritability, ii. 58 stimulation of nerve, ii. 25 tonus, ii. 1 13 Chemistry of nerves, ii. 169 ( Ihemotaxis, ii. 166 influence of, on neuroblasts, ii. 176 • b.emo-1 ropism, ii. 166 Chest, effects of opening, i. 115 • Ihest-voice, ii. 132 Cheyne-Stokes respiration, i. 124 chief cells of the gastric glands, i. '.'.''.7 ( hinese wax. i. 540 < Ihinolin, i 571 Chloral, effect of, on the respiratory rhythm, i. 125 hydrate, i. 536 Chlorine, inhalation of. i. 1 111 occurrence of, i. 507 i Shlorocruorin, i. 578 < Ihloroform, action of, on coagulation of muscle plasma, ii 164 effect of, on nerve-currents, ii. L56 Chloroform, fate of, in the body, i. 533 vapor, action of, on nerves, ii. 60 Chocolate, nutritive value of, i. 357 Cholagogues, i. 2 16 Cholesterin, i. 575 amount of, in the blood, i. 51 disl ribution of, i. 325 e.xcret ion of, i. 325 in nerve, ii. L69 of bile, i. 245 of milk, i. •.'ill Of sebaceous secretion, i. 257 < boletelin, i. 57 1 Cholin, i. 511, r,4:5 Cholo-lnematin, i. 323 Chondroitic acid, i. 578 < 'homl ro-mucoid, i. 578 Chorda t\ in pa 11 i nerve, gustatory function of, ii. llll vaso-dilator function of, i. 194 Chorda- tcndineie, i. 109 Chorion, ii. IT", frondosum, ii. 474 laeve, ii. 17 I Chorionic fluid, ii. 473 villi, ii. 4?:; Chromatic aberration, ii. 316 Chromatin, i. 22, 28 Chromatoblasts of pleuronectidae, ii. 20 ( 'hromo-proteids, i. 576 Chromosomes, i. 22, 28 number of, in the segmentation nucleus, ii. 466 of germ-cells, hereditary function of, ii. 499 of human spermatozoa, ii. 443 of the sexual elements, reduced number of, ii. 4. ".4 ovarian, changes in, during maturation, ii. 451 number of, ii. 450 reduction of, in maturation of spermatozoa, ii. 445 Chronograph, description of, ii. 100 Chyme, i. 287, 381 Ciliary ganglion, ii. 323 muscles, action of, in accommodation, ii. 309 nerves, long, ii. 324 short, ii. 311, 323 Circulating proteid, definition of, i. 346 Circulation, artificial, through isolated organs, ii. 68 capillary, velocity of, i. 83 cerebral, i. 203 of bydriodic acid. i. 509 of hydrofluoric acid. i. 510 of the bile, i. :;•.':;. 324 of the blood, causes of, i. 77 definition of, i. 76 discovery of, i. 7t> in the retina, ii. 322 microscopic appearances of, i. 80 portal, i. 77 pulmonary, i. 78, 103 of the brain ami cord, ii. 286 rate of. i. 79, 98 pulmonary, i. 103 renal, i. 255 Circulal ion-time. i. 79 Circumduction, movement of. ii. 416 Climacteric, ii. 159, 190 ovulation after, ii. loll Climate, influence of, on age of puberty, ii. 489 on bodj temperal a re, i. 169 on time of climacteric, ii. 490 < litoris. ii. 462 homology of, ii. in 1 Clothing, influence of. on heat-loss, i. 486 CKXFJiAL f.XDF.X. 529 Clotting of blood, i. 55 of milk, i. 295 Clupein, i. 580 CO; elimination, cutaneous, i. 258, 342 during muscular wmk, i. 361 sleep, i. 361 Coagulated proteids, properties of, i. 578 Coagulating enzymes, definition of, i. 280 Coagulation of the blood, accelerating agents of, i. 61 conditions necessary for, i. 57 description of, i. 54 intravascular, i. 60 nature of, i. 60 retarding influences affecting, i. 61, 62 theories of, i. 55, 56 time taken by, i. 55 uses of, i. 55 of milk, i. 295 of muscle-plasma, action of drugs on, ii. 164 of myosin, ii. 163 Cocaine, action of, on conductivity in nerves, ii. 93 on the tongue, ii. 413 effect of, on intestinal movements, i. 384 Cochlea, anatomy of, ii. 374 bony, ii. 372 membranous, structure of, ii. 376 Cochlear root of the auditory nerve, central paths of, ii. 237 Coefficient of absorption of liquids for gases, i. 414 Coffee, nutritive value of, i. 357 stimulating action of, ii. 75 Cold and warm points of the skin, ii. 398 effect of, on coagulation of the blood, i. 61 Collagen, i. 580 Collaterals of axones, ii. 173 Colloid, i. 578 substance of the thyroid, secretion of, 268 Color of objects, relation of, to intensity of il- lumination, ii. 333 sensations in indirect vision, ii. 333 phenomena of, ii. 333 theories, ii. 335 triangle, ii. 334 vision, theories of, ii. 335 Color-blindness, ii. 338 hereditary transmission of, ii. 494 of the rods, ii. 342 Colored shadows from simultaneous contrast, ii. 347 Color-mixture, ii. 333 Colors, binocular combination of, ii. 358 complementary, ii. 334 physical basis for, ii. 332 relative luminosity of, ii. 340 saturation of, ii. 342 Colostrum corpuscles, origin of, i. 263 definition of, i. 26 1 Combinational tunes, ii. 3*7 ( 'onibined proteids. i. 579 Combustion, i. 501 equivalent of foods, i. 365 Comedones, i. 257 Commissure, Meynert's. ii. 23* von Gudden's, ii. 238 Commissures, origin of, from central cells, ii.205 Common sensation, definition of, ii. 399 sensibility, afferent paths of the nerves of, ii. 230 Commutators, method of using, ii. 36 Complemental air, i. 427 Complementary colors, ii- 334 Composite tones, analysis of, ii 384 Compressed air, respiration of, i. 452 Conceptions, multiple, ii. 1*2 ::i Concha of the external ear. ii. 362 Condiments, nutritive value of, i. 359 ( londucl ion by contiguity, ii. 81 directions of, ii. 84 from neurone to neurone, ii. 84 in branching nerves, ii. bO in ganglion-cells, ii. 97 in muscles, ii. 80 in nerve-trunks, ii. 79 in ner\ es, effects of, ii. 95 in the heart of the contraction wave. i. 154 of nerve-impulses, direction of, ii. 184 from neurone to neurone, ii. 207 process, nature of, ii. 97 rate of, ii. 87 Conductivity, action of drugs on, ii. 93 definition of, ii. 20, 77 dependence of, on protoplasmic continuity, ii. 77 effect of constant current on, ii. 94 influences affecting, ii. 91 of living matter, i. 21 of muscle, ii. 20 of nerves, ii. 21 of neurone, ii. 189 of ova, ii. 22 Condyloid joints, ii. 416 Cones, retinal, function of, ii. 341 movements of, ii. 331 Contluxion in space perception, ii. 353 Congenita] resemblances, ii. 494 varial ions, ii. ."><>() Congo-red test for mineral acids, i. 289 Conin, action of, on end-plates, ii. 27 Conjugate foci in a dioptric system, ii. 302 Conjugated sulphates, nutritive, history of, i. 340 ( lonjugal ion, ii. 440 Consciousness, i. 29 cerebral origin of, ii. 172 Consonants, ii. 436 Constant current, contracture effect of, in mus- cles, ii. 131 effect of, on conductivity, ii. 94 on muscles, ii. 61 on nerves, ii. 62 Constrictor nerves of the iris, ii. 323 Continuous contractions, ii. 127 Contractility, definition of, ii. 17, 98 in vorticella, ii. 20 occurrence of, ii. 20 of amoebae, ii. L9 of as1 ral lays. ii. 470 of living mat ler. i. 21 of muscle, ii. 17 adaptation of, to their normal functions, ii. Id* of ova. ii. 22 of plain muscle, i 370 Contraction curve <>f muscle, effect of frequent excital ions on, ii. 1 1"> idio muscular, ii. 92 of muscles, post-mortem, ii. 160 relation of, to structure, ii. IC7 remainder, ii. 106 volume of t lie heart, i 105 wave in muscle, rate of transmission of, ii. B7 length of, ii 88 of the heart, rate of propagation of, i. 153 Contractions from repeated single stimuli, ii. 1 12 inl roductory, ii. 113 isomet lie, ii 110 isotonic, ii. 110 normal, tetanic nature of, ii 132 of rigor caloris, ii 1 65 Contracture after frequenl excitations, ii. 128 after single excitation, ii. 129 definition of, ii- 1 1<> 530 ( } EX ERA L INDEX. Contracture from fatigne, ii. 130 in dying muscles, ii. 132 in rigor mortis, ii. 128 in veratria poisoning, ii. 128 normal, ii. L29 hi aeck muscles after cerebellar injury, ii. 272 pathological, ii. 127, 132 relation of, to tetanus, ii. 117, 122, 124 Contractures, ii. 12? ( 'mit rast, ii. 346 in spare perception, ii. 352 Convergence of the eyes in accommodation, ii. 311 muscular mechanism of, ii. 300 Co-ordination of the efferenl impulses in re- flexes, ii. 214 Copulation, ii. 463 ( lore-conductors, ii. 158 Cornea, curvature of, ii. 303 Corniculum Laryngis, ii. 425 Cornutine, action of, on muscles, ii. 137 Corona radiata, ii. 454 of the ovum, ii. 450 Coronary arteries, anatomy of, i. 179, 180 ligation of, i. 181, 183 circulation, effect of ventricular systole on, i. 185 volume of, i. 1- I veins, closure of, i. 184 Corpora Arautii. i. 112 cavernosa of 1 he penis, ii. 448 striata, fund ions of, ii. 271 Corpus callosum, ('unctions of, ii. 270 luteum, ii. 455 spongiosum of the penis, ii. 448 Corpuscles, colostrum, i. 263 ot' t lie blood, i. 45 salivary, i. 283 Corresponding points of the retinas, ii. :;.">!( Cortex cerebri, connection of, with the respira- tory centre, i. 463 effects of localized electrical stimulation of, ii. 211 electrical stimulation of, ii. 242 number of nerve-cells in. ii. 2*1 course of efferent impulses from, ii. 251 latent areas of, ii. 261 Corti, cells of, ii. 377 organ of, structure of, ii. 377 rods of, ii. ; :77 Cortical anas. ii. 243 motor, in man. ii. 250 size of, ii. 247 centre-, ii. 243 motor control, crossed, ii. 251 multiple character of, ii. 250 regions, ii. 2 13 stimulation, inhibitory effects of, ii 221 vascular effects of, i. 202 Costal respiration, definition of, i. 398 ( Soughing, i. 154 Coughs, sympathetic, i. 155 < o\\ per'- gland, ii. 1 13 histology of, ii. 148 secret ion ol'. ii. t 16 Crab-extract, Lymphagogic action of, i. 73 <'rab-, regeneration of lost parts in, ii. 196 < Irania I nerves, afferent, ii. 236 Creatin, chemical constitution of, i. 550 in muscle, ii. 166 nutritive history of, i. 339, 551 Creatinin, i. 551 nutritive history of. i. 339 of muscle, ii. 1C7 Cre.-ol. i. 569 elimination, i. 340 Cretinism, sporadic, ii. 280 Crico-arytenoid muscle, lateral, ii. 426 posterior, ii. 126 Cricoid cartilage, ii. I2"> ( 'rico-t liyroid muscles, ii. 426 Criminals, weight of the brain in, ii. 277 Crista acustiea of the semicircular canals, ii.373 Critical period of nerves, ii. 66 Crossed cerebral circulation, i. 443 Cross-suturing of nerve-trunks, ii. 201 Cruciate heat-centre, ii. 271 Crying, i. 454 ( i\ stalloids, diffusion of, i. 69 Crystal- of CO-hsemoglobin, i. 40 of ha-niiii, i. 44, 573 of haemoglobin, i. :i;» ( tupola of the cochlea, ii. 375 ( 'ura re, action of, ii. 26 Currents of action in muscle, ii. 150 in nerves, ii. 153 of rest. ii. 1 17 theories as to their cause, ii. 148 Curve of fatigue, with repeated single contrac- tions, ii. 113 of intensity of sleep, ii. 294 of muscle contraction, effect of frequent ex- cital ions on, ii. 1 15 of muscular contractions, ii. 100 of work for muscles, ii. 140 Cutaneous nerves, influence of, on respiration, i. 163 respiration, i. 422 secretion, i. 257 sensations, cortical area for, ii. 253 disturbance of, in disease, ii. 403 varieties of, ii. 390 temperature points, ii, 398 Cyanamide, i. 5 12 ( lyanogen gas, i. 541 inhalation, i. 440 ( 'ynurenic acid, i. 571 ( lystein, i. 546 Cystin, i. 517 Cytology, definition of, i. 31 Cytoplasmic changes in nerve-cells, ii. 182 Cytosin, i. 579 " Dangerous region," i. 97 Daniel] cell, ii. 28 Darwin's theory of heredity, ii. 501 Death, definition of, ii. 491 of the tissues, ii. 192 somatic, ii. 491 theory of. ii. 192 Decidua graviditatis, ii. 461, 471 menstrualis, ii. 15S, 45 of soluble substances, i. 69 Diapedesis of maternal leucocytes into the foetus, ii. 476 Diaphoretics, effect of, on heat dissipation, i. 489 Diaphragm, movements of, i. 398 Diarthrosis, ii. 415 Diastase, i. 280 Diastatic enzymes, i. 280, 566 Diaxonic nerve-cells, ii. 178 Dicrotic pulse, i. 1 1 1 wave of the pulse-curve, i. 143 Diet, accessory articles of, i.357 average, for man, i. 366 Dietetics, i. 366 Differential manometer, i. 131 tones, ii. 387 Diffusion, definition of, i. 65 of central nerve-impulses, ii. 208 of impulses in the cord, influences affecting, ii. 217 of nerve-impulse, peripheral, ii. 218 of proteids, i. 70 through membranes, i. 66 Digastric muscle, i. 372; ii. 426 Digestion, action of alcohol on, i. 535 gastric, i. 287 influence of, on respiratory exchanges, i. 431 intestinal, i. 299 in the large intestine, i. 309 of fats, i. 305 of proteids, i. 292, 301 of starch, i 284 pancreatic, i. :;ioptry, definition of, ii. 304 Dioxyacetone, i. 558 Dioxyphenyl-acetic acid, i. 570 1 diphasic current of action, i. L52 Direction, judgments of, by means of auditory sensations, ii. 389 of the nerve-impulse, ii. 1-1 Disaccharides, i. 56 1 digestion of, i. 308 Disassimilation, definition of, i. l!t Discord, ii. :>1 Discriminating sensibility of the skin for pres- sure, ii. :;!»-.' Discriminative sensibility for difference of tem- perature, ii. 397 Discus proligerus, ii. 450, 454 Diseases, inheritance of, ii. 498 Dispermy, ii. 471 Dispersion of light, ii. 316 Dissociation of electrolytes, i. 67 of the axial and focal adjustments of the eye, ii. 312 Distance, judgments of sensation by means of auditory, ii. 389 perception of, ii. 354 visual perception of, ii. 348 Disuse, effect of, on muscles, ii. 77 i Sureties, action of, i. 254 Dizziness, ii. 105 Dogs, removal of cerebrum in, ii. 267 Domestication, effect of, on menstruation in animals, ii. 160. 462 Dorsal nerve-roots, efferent fibres in, ii. 203 roots, degeneration resulting from section of, ii. 227 spinal nerve-roots, number of fibres of, ii. 230 Dreams, ii. 293 Drinking-water, i. 504 Dropsy, i. 147 Drowning, phenomena of, i. 445 resuscital ion from, i. 1 15 Drugs, action of, on body-temperature, i. 472 on salivary glands, i. 222, 229 on sweat-glands, i. -jilil on thermogenesis, i. 18 1 on thermolysis, i. 189 Du Bois-Eeymond's key, ii. 30 law of st iinulat ion. ii. .".-.' theory of currents of rest, ii. 148 Duct of Bartholin, i. 217 of Rivinus, i. 217 of Stenson, i. 217 of Wharton, i. '.'17 of Wirsung, i. 231 Ductus cochlearis, structure of. ii. :;7l endol vmphal icus. ii. ,",7-'! venosus of t he embryo, ii. I7n' Duration of electric currents, effeel of. mi their irritating power, ii. 16 Dynamic equilibrium, organs of, ii. L07 Dyslysin. i. 511 Dyspepsia accompanying the climacteric, ii. I'M) cause of, i. 309 I >\ spncea, definition of. i. l li effeel of, on t he iris, ii. 32 I on intest inal \ ements, i. 386 phenomena of, i. 1 1 1 varieties of. i. 1 13, I 1 I Ea.b, analysis of composite tones by, ii. 384 anatomy of. ii. 362 discriminative sensibility of, for pitch, ii. :;<, lai Lgue of, ii. :;s7 imperfeel ion- of, ii. 3SS 532 GENERAL INDEX. Ear, membranous labyrinth of, ii. 372 ossicles of, ii. 365 sensibility of, in perception of time intervals, ii. 388 Earth-worms, regeneration of lost parts iu, ii. 4! Mi Ecfc fistula, i. 336 Edestine, i. 577 Efferenl fibres of the optic nerves, ii. 240 impulse in reflexes, co-ordination of, ii. 214 neurones of the dorsal spinal nerve-roots, ii. 203 of the spinal cord, ii. 203 * paths from the cortex, course of, ii. 244 respiratory nerves, i. 163 Egg albumin, absortion of, i. 315 Ejaculation, ii. 165 Ejaculatory duct. ii. I IT Elasticity of muscle, ii. 105 Elastin, i. 580 Electric currents, correlation of their duration with histological structures, ii. 47 effect of, duntt ion of, ii. 46 on muscles, ii. 61 on nerves, ii. 62 their density, ii. 41 galvanic, effect of, on normal human nerves, ii. 51 influence of their direction in nerves, ii. 48 varying duration of, ii. 47 methods of detecting, ii. 145 spread of, in moist conductors, ii. 41 stimulating effect of, ii. 28 organs, ii. 145 Electrical changes iii active glands, i. 231 in the beating heart, i. 152, 153 in the heart during vagus stimulation, i. 164 in the retina, ii. 331 phenomena of muscle and nerve, ii. 144 of nerves, interpretation of, ii. 15S stimulation of nerve, ii. 25 of nerves, law of, ii. 32 Electrodes, shielded, ii. 41 varieties of, ii. 29 Electrolytes, definition of, i. 67 Electrostatic changes, stimulating action of, ii. 42 Electrotonic changes of conductivity, ii. 50 of irritability, ii. til in human nerves, ii. 65 twitch, ii. 157 Electrotonus, ii. 62 Embryo, nutrition of, ii 175 rate of growth of, ii. 487 Emigration of leucocytes, i. 83 Emmetropia, ii. 313 Emmetropic eye, ii. 312 Emphysema, influence of, on the respiratory rhythm, i. 424 Emulsification of Eats, i. 306 influence of the bile on, i. 307 Emulsions, preparation of, i. - 1< >T . 559 Encephala, classification of, according to weight. ii. 275 Encephalon, specific gravity of, ii. 275 weight of, ii. 274, 275 End-bulbs, sensory, ii. 392 Endocardiac pressure. See Intracardiac pressure. Endolymph, ii. 372 End-organs, importance of, in touch sensations, ii. 396 transmission of excitation by means of, ii. 82 Enemata, nutritive, i. 315 Energy liberated in contracting muscles, ii. 138 potential, of foods, i. 364 Engelmann's theory of the nature of muscular contraction, ii. 105 Environment, influence of, on organisms, ii. 493 Enzyme action, theories of, i. 282 glycolyl ic, i. 354 Enzymes, classification of, i. 280 composition of, i. 279 definition of, i. 27!i effect of, on blood coagulation, i. 63 genera] properties of, i. 281 nn.de of action of. i. 282 of pancreatic juice, i. 232, 235, 301 solubility of, i. 281 Eosinophiles, i. 47 Epididymis, ii. 447 Epigenesis, theory of, ii. 500,504 Epiglottis, ii. 421 Epiguanin, i. 554 Epinephrin, i. 272, 572 action of, on muscles, ii. 138 Episarcin, i. 554 Equilibrium of the body, definition of, ii. 404 relation of the cerebellum to, ii. 273 sense of, ii. 1<>4 Erect ion, ii. 464 of t he heart, i. 114 spinal centre for, ii. 464 lire, tor clitoridis muscle, ii. 464 penis, action of, in erection, ii. 464 muscles, ii. 1 19 Hreetores spime muscles, respiratory action of, i. 405 Erythroblasts, i. 45 Erythrodextrin, i. 285, 566 Erythrose, i. 562 Escape of the heart from vagus inhibition, i. 163 Eserin, action of, on nerves and muscles, ii. 60 Ether, action of, on coagulation of muscle- plasma, ii. 164 on conductivity of nerves, ii. 93 effect of, on nerve-currents, ii. 155 ethyl, i. 536 vapor, action of, on nerves, ii. 60 Ethereal sulphates, i. 506 of the urine, i. 572 Ethers, properties of, i. 536 Ethyl alcohol, i. 535 El li\ la in i tie, i. 541 Eudiometer, i. 421 Eupnoea, definition of, 440 Eustachian tube, ii. :;<;:; function of, ii. 369 Excitability, changes in, during Walleriau de- generation, ii. 69 Excitation, cardiac, electrical variation in, i. 153 propagat ion of, i. 153, 154 wave, cardiac, i. 152 Excretin, occurrence of, in feces, i. 320 Excretions, definition of, i. 213 Exercise, effect of. on growth, ii. 489 on metabolism, i. 359 on muscular endurance, ii. 76 on pulse-rate, i. 121 Exhaustion of muscles, ii. 72 Expiration, forced, muscles of, i. 407 movements id', i. 406 Expiratory centre, i. 157 Explosive consonants, ii 137 Extensibility of muscle, ii. 105 External auditory meatus, ii. 362 ear, anatomy of. ii. 362 rectus muscle, ii 299 Extirpation of the liver, i. 336 of t in- pancreas, i. 266 of the thyroids, i. 268 Extractives, nitrogenous, of muscle, ii. 166 of t he blood, i. 50, 51 Ext tacts, adrenal, i. 271 ovarian, i. 271 GENERAL J XI > EX. 533 Extracts, testicular, i. 273 thyroid, i. 269 Extrapolar region, ii. 62 Exudations, secretion of, i- 215 Eye, abnormal positions of, alter cerebellar in- jury, ii. 272 adaptation of, to light, ii. 326 axes of rotation of, ii. 299 chromatic aberration of, ii. 316 constants, chauges in, during accommodatiou, ii. 311 defects in the dioptric apparatus of, ii. 314 dioptric apparatus of, ii. 300 mechanical movements of, ii. 298 movements, binocular co-ordination in, ii. 300 extent of, ii. 298 muscles of, ii. 299 innervation of, ii. 300 optical constants of, ii. 303 power of, ii 3<»1 positions of, ii. 299 refractive media of, ii. 302 surfaces of, ii. 303 spherical aberration of. ii. 315 Fallopian tubes, ii. 443, 456 False amnion, ii. 473 Falsetto register of the voice, ii. 433 Far-point of vision, ii. 312 Fat, affinity of cell-substance for, i. 5(58 nutritive history of, ii. 559 origin of, from carbohydrates, i. 352 from proteid, i. 351, 560 Fat-absorption, influence of bile on, i. 325 mechanism of, i. 318 Fat-combustion, equivalent of, i. 365 Fat-formation in the body, i. 351, 560 Fatigue, cerebral anaemia from, ii. 288 curve with repeated single contraction, ii. 113 effect of, on height of contraction, ii. 113 on muscular contraction, ii. 130 on rigor caloris, ii. 165 mortis, ii. 160 from voluntary muscular contraction, ii. 134 in nerve-fibres, ii. 195 of central nervous system, ii. 289 of motor end-organs, ii. 83 of muscle, ii. 66, 70 recovery from, ii. 73 of nerve-cells, ii. 136, 191 of nerves, ii. 75, 96 of retina, ii. 344 relation of, to sleep, ii. 291 theories of. ii. 72 to auditory sensations, ii. 387 Fat-metabolism, acetone formation in, i 537 Fats, absorption of, in the stomach, i. 313 action of. on gastric secretion, i '.'ll digestion of, i. 305 dynamic value of. i. 175 emulsification of, i. 306 gastric digestion of, i. 297 nutritive value of, i. 277, 350 of feces, i. 319 of muscle, ii. 167 Origin of, in the body, i. 351. 560 relation of, to glycogen format ion. i. 329 to muscular work, ii. 71 synthesis of, from fatty acids, i. 558 Fatty acids, monobasic, i. 532 degeneration in phosphorus-poisoning, i. 51 l Feces, composil ion of. i. 319 Fellic acid, i. 543 Female births, relative number of, ii. \-''> pronucleus, ii. 453 Females, rate of growth in, ii. 4*8 Fenestra ovalis, ii. 363 Fenestra rotunda, ii. 363. 375 use of, ii. 376 Ferment, myosinogen-coagulating, ii. 161 Fermentation, alcoholic, i. 535 lactic, i. 545 Ferment--, unorganized, i. 279 Ferratin, i. 528, 529 Ferric phosphates, i 528 Ferrosulphide, i. 528 Fertilization, ii. 466 Fetal membranes, ii. 172 Fever, body-temperature in, i. 472 cause of, i. 473 effect of, on blood coagulation, i. 55 on the respiratory centre, i. 458 heat dissipation in, i. 489 Fibrillar contraction of the heart, i. 181. l-'J Fibrin fermeut, i. 56 absence of, iu circulating blood, i. 61 nature of, i. 57 origin of, i. 59 preparation of, i. 59 mode of deposition of, i. 54, 55 Fibrin-globulin, i. 56 Fibrinogen, i. 53. 54 Fibrinoplastin, i. 56 Fictitious meal, effect of, on gastric secretion, i. 239 Field of vision, binocular rivalry of. ii. 358 Filtration processes in secretion, i. 213, 215 Fimbria? of the Fallopian tube, ii. 456 Fish, bonv, removal of cerebral hemispheres in, ii. 263 semicircular canals in, ii. 4(>7 visual accommodation in, ii. 306 Flavors, nutritive value of. i. 359 Flicker photometry, ii. 345 Fluorine, occurrence of, i. 510 Focal illumination of the eye, ii. 320 Foci, conjugate, ii. 302 principal, ii. 302 Food, combust ion equivalent of, i. 365 definif ion of, i. 275 dynamic value of. i. 364 effect of, on respiratory activity, i. 431 energy liberated by, i. 171 influence of, on thermogenesis, i. 484 rate of movement of. in the intestines, i. 31 I Food-stuffs, classification of, i. 276 composition of, i. 278 Liebig's classification of, i. •"> 16 Foramen ovale of the foetal heart, ii. 476 Force of ventricular systole during vagus stim- ulation, i. 163 Forced movements after section of the semicir- cular canal-, ii. 405 in frogs, ii. 266 Formic acid, i. 534 aldehyde, i. 533 Formose, synthesis of, i. 533 Fovea cent ralis, ii. 327 Franklin's theory of Color vision, ii. .'!.".7 Frequency of respiration, conditions affecting, i. 125 relation of, to the pulse-rate, i. 126 Frict ionals. ii. 137 Frogs, removal of cerebral hemispheres in, ii. 264 striked muscle of, time of single contraction in. ii. 108 Frontal lobes of the hemispheres, effect of re- moval of, ii. 262 Fuhlspare, cortical, ii. 252 Fundamental tone, definition of, ii. 383 Gal ictose, L 562. 564 Gall-bladder, motor nerves of, i. :i- 534 < } i:\KIi. 1 /. INDEX. Galvani, Luigi, ii. 28 Galvanic current, action of, ou conductivity, ii. 94 contracture effect of, on muscles, ii. 131 effect of, on heart apex, i. 150 on muscles, ii. * : I mi nerves, ii. 62 of making and breaking, ii. 31 on normal human nerves, ii. 51 opening and closing contractions with, ii. 38 Galvanometers, ii. l l"> GalvanOtonus, ii. 54, 131 I kunogenesis, ii. 140 Ganglion spirale of the ear, ii. 376 submaxillary, i. 219 Ganglion-cells, conduction in, ii. 97 of the heart, i. 1 18 Gas analysis, i. 421 Gaseous exchanges in the brain, ii. 288 interchanges in the lungs, i. 410, 417 in the tissues, i. 419 Gas-pump, description of, i. 42U Gases, absorption of. i. 414 in the blood, respiratory changes in, i. 411 in the large intestine, i. 320 law of partial pressure of, i. 413 of muscle, ii. 168 of the saliva, i. 221 poisonous, inhalation of, i. 440 solutions of, i. 415 Gastric digestion of proteids, i. 292 fistulse, i. 288 glands, histology of, i. 237 secretory changes in, i. 242 value of, i. 299 juice, acidity of, i. 289 action of, on carbohydrates, i. 296 on milk, i. 296 antiseptic property of, i. 288 artificial, preparation of, i. 291 composition of, i. 238, 288 methods of obtaining, i. 287 mineral constituents of, i. 530 secretion, inhibition of, i. 241 nervous regulation of. i. 239 normal mechanism of, i. 240 relation of, to the character of the diet, i. 241 stimulants for, i. 241 Gelatin, digestion of, in the stomach, i. 297 nutritive value of, i. 349 proteid, protecting power of, i. 567 < lelatoses, i. 297 Geminal fibres of the pyramidal tracts, ii. 245 Gemmules of the germ-plasma, ii. 499 Genio-hyoid muscle, ii. 126 function of, in mastication, i. 372 Gerhardt's reaction, i ."37 Germinal spot of the ovary, ii. 450 transmission of infectious diseases, ii. 498 vesicle, al ructure of, ii. 450 Germ-plasm as a basis of heredity, ii. 499 coin inuity of, ii. 502 definition of, ii- 196 morphological nature of, ii. 499 origin of, ii. I!'!' Gestation, duration of, ii. 478 i Hand, adrenal, i. 271 mammary, i. 262 pancreatic, i. 231, 266 parathyroid, i. 268 parotid, i. 217 sublingual, i 217 submaxillary, i. 217 thyroid, i. 267 Gland-cells, electric currents in. ii 145 selective activity of, i. 27 Glands, albuminous, histology of, i. 216 Brunner's, i. 243 cutaneous, i. 257 gasi ric, i. 237 intesl tnal, i. 243 Lieberkiihn's, i. 243 mucous, histology of, i. 216 of Bartholin, ii. 462 of Littrc. ii. 448 salivary, i. 215 sebaceous, i. 257 serous, definition of, i. 216 structure of, i. 211 sweat, i. 259 I Hans penis, ii. 449 Glauber's salt, i. 522 Gliding movements in joints, ii. 416 Gb.bin. i. 37 Globulicidal action of serum, i. 36 < rlobulins, i. 577 Glomeruli, renal, secretory function of, i. 253 Glossopharyngeal nerve, gustatory function of, ii. 410 nerves, influence of, on respiration, i. 462 Glossopharvngeus, central conduction paths for. ii. *236 Glottis, ii. 423 tedenia of, ii. 422 respiratory movements of, i. 408 ; ii. 429 Glucosamin. i. 564 Glucoses, i. 562 S3 nthesis of, i. 563 Glutamic acid, i. 558 Glutamin, i. 558 Glutolin, i. 53 Glutoses, i. 297 Glycerin, i. 558 aldehyde, i. 558 phosphoric acid, i. 559 Glycerose, i. 558 Glyeocoll. i. 537. 543 in muscles, ii. 107 nutritive history of, i. 538 Glycogen, i. 566 amount of, in the liver, i. 327 demonstration of, in the liver, i. 327 distribution of. i. 330 effect of exercise on, i. 361 of starvation on, i. 362 of sugars on. i. 328 function of. i. 329 of muscles, i. 330; ii. 167 origin of. i. 326, 327 properties of, i 327, 566 Glycogen-elimination of the liver, i. 265 Glycogen-formation, effect of proteid diet on. i. 328 Gl\ cogeii-formers, i. 32* Glycogenic theory, i. 329 Glycolysis, i. 354 Glycolytic enzyme, i. 280, 354 origin of, i. 2(17 Glyco-proteids, i. 576, 578 ( Hycosazones, i. 562 i Hyco secretory nerves, i. 248 Gl> coses, i. 562 Glycosuria after pancreas extirpation, i. 266, 563 Glycuronic acid, i. 567 Gmelin's test fur bile-pigments, i. 322, 574 Goblet ■•ells. i. -.'Hi Goitre, i. 269 Golgi, organ of. in tendons, ii. 402 Gout, i. 557 ( Graafian follicles, ii. 154 Grammeter, i. 477 Gram-molecular solution, i. 67 CKXERAL I XD EX. 535 Graphic method of studying muscular con- tractions, ii. 99 Gravity, influence of, on cerebral circulation, ii. 287 Gray matter of the cerebrum, water contents of. ii. 271 Growth alter birth, ii. 487 before birth, ii. 186 increase of fibres of the cortex during, ii. 282 of functional neurones during, ii. 282 influence of sex on I lie rate of, ii. 4S8 influences which modify, ii. 4s>L> of nerve-cells, ii. L76 Guanidin, i. 550 Guanin, i. 339, 554 Gunzburg's reagent, i. 508 Gustatory nerves, ii. 410 sensations, ii. 411 Guttural consonants, ii. 437 Gyniueinra silvestre, action of, on taste-nerves, ii. 413 H.emati.v, i. 37, 44, 573 Hsematogen, i. 356 composition of, i. 579 nutritive value of, i. 528 Haeinatoidin, i. 44, 323, 574 Hsematopoiesis, definition of, i. 45 Hematopoietic tissues, embryonic, i. 46 Hsematoporphyrin, i. 44, 574 Hsnierythrin, i. 578 Hsemin, i. 44, 573 Hpemochromogen, i. 37, 44, 573 Hreniocyanin, i. 578 Haemoglobin, i. 573 absorption spectra of, i. 43 action of, on carbonates, i. 517 affinity of, for CO2, i. 417 amount of, i. 38 compounds of, with gases, i. 38 condition of, in the corpuscles, i. 35 crystals of, i. 39 decomposition products of, i. 37 derivatives of, i. 44 distribution of, in animals, i. 37 elementary composition of, i. 37 molecular formula of, i. 37, 38 nature of, i. 37 of muscle-serum, ii. 166 oxygen capacity of, i. 416 Hair-eel Is of the crista acustica, ii. 374 of the organ of < lorti, ii- :!77 Hamulus, ii. -iTii Harmonic overtones, ii. 386 Harmony, ii. 387 Hawking, i. 151 Head register of the voice, ii. 433 vaso-motor nerves of, i. 204 Hearing, ii. 362 keenness of, ii. .'!71 relation of, to speech, ii. 431 Heart, anaemia of, i. 1 33 artificial stimulation of, i. 156 augmentor nerves of, i. 167 cause of rhythmic heat of, i. 148 cent ripetal nerves of. i. 171 changes in, due to pregnancy, ii. 477 in form of. i. 111! in position of, i. 114 in si/.e of, i. 1 12 compensatory pause of, i. 156 diphasic act ion iai rrente in. ii. L52 electrical currents of, i. 152 erection of. i. 1 1 I fibrillar emit rait ion of, i. 181 heat produced by, i. L08 human, output of, i. Kit! Heart, intrinsic nerves of, i. 148 isolation of, i. 1 I-. 1 -? ; ii 69 lymphatics of, i. 186 muscle, atrophy of, after section of the vagi, i. L67 conduction of the contraction wave l>y, i l" 1 normal stimulus of, i. 151 rate of conduction in, ii. 89 rhythmicity of, i. 151 rigor mortis of, ii. 162 nutrition of, i. 179 position of, i. 117 pumping action of, i. 78 refractory period of, i. 156 suction-pump action of, i. 134 tetanus of, i. L65 vaso-motor nerves of, i. 206 work done by, i. 1U7 Heart-beat, abnormal sequence of, i. 152 conduction of, from auricles to ventricles, i. 155 effect of blood-supply on, i. 186 genesis of, i. 149. 150 heat produced by, i. 108 rate of. i. 121 voluntary control of, ii. 214 Heart-pause, i. 122 Heart-sounds, i. 118 Heat, expenditure of, i. 476 income of, i. 175 source of, i. 474 Heat-centres, ii. 271 Heat-dissipation, conditions affecting, i. 485 estimation of, i. 480 Heat-dyspnoea, i. 1 11, 443 Heat-production, amount of, i. 364 by the heart, i. 108 conditions affecting, i. 482 estimation of, i. 4M in contracting muscles, ii. 138 in muscles, ii. 142 in nerves, ii. 96 in rigor mortis, ii. 160 relation of, to respiratory activity, i. 483 Heat-rays of ether, ii. .'Ilil Heat -regulation, i. 195 Height of contraction, dependence of, on the load, ii. Ill effect of temperature on, ii. 136 Helico-proteid, composition of, i. .".?ii Helicotrema, ii. ::7<> Hemianopsia, anatomical hasis for, ii. 240 from cortical lesions, ii. 255 Hemi-peptone, decomposition of, by trypsin, i. 303 definition of, i. 293 Hemisections of the cord alternating at differ- ent levels, ii. 230 Brown-Sequard's paralysis from, ii. •.':::; degeneration resulting from, ii. 228 effect of, in man, ii. '.'.;:; on vriivat ion and mot ion, ii. }30 in animals, ii. 234 physiological effeel of. ii. -.':;i Hemorrhage, effeel of. on hematopoiesis, i. It! fatal limits of, i. liii regeneration of the hi 1 after, i. 63 reiat ion of, to blood-pressure, i. 91 saline injections after, i. ill Hemorrhagic dyspnoea, i. ill Hepatin, 1. 528 Heredity, definition of. ii. pi;; physical hasis of, i. 28 theories of. ii. 198 Bering's theory of color vision, ii. 336 Hermann's theory of currents of rest, ii. 1 18 Heteromita, reproducl ion in. ii. 1 10 536 GENEBAL INDEX. Hexou-bases, origin of, i. 580 Hexoses, i. 562 Hibernation, effect of, ou the respiratory quo- tient, i. 438 Biccough, i 155 Higher brain-centres for the beart, i. 17b Hinge-joints, ii. 416 Hippuric acid, nutritive history of, i. 339 Histidin, i. 552 1 1 istohtematin, i. 44, 578 Histology of striped muscle, ii. 104 Histon, i. 580 t-ll't'ci of, cm intravascular clotting, i. 61 Hofacker-Sadhr law, ii. 484 Holmgren method for testing color visiou, ii. 339 Homogciitisic acid, i. 570 Homotbermous animals, i. 407 Horopter, ii. '.'>'<'.> Hiifner's method of urea determination, i. 549 Human muscles, fatigue of, with artificial stim- ulation, ii. 134 Hunger, ii. 101 Hunger-centre, clinical evidence for, ii. 404 Hydra, regeneration of lost parts in, ii. 496 Hydremia from saline injections, i. 69 Hydremic plethora, effect of, on lymph secre- tion, i. 74 Hydration, nature of the process of, i. 503 Hydriodic acid. i. 509 Hydrobilirabin, i. 320 Hydrobromic acid. i. 509 Hydrocarbons, saturated, i. 531 Hydrochloric acid, occurrence of, i. 507 of the gastric juice, i. 238 preparation of, i. 507 properties of, i. 508 secretion of, i. 289 tests for. i. 508 Hydrocumaric acid. i. 570 Hydrocyanic acid. i. 54'.' action of, on coagulation of muscle-plasma, ii. 164 Hydrofluoric acid, circulation of, in the body, i. 510 Hydrogen, inhalation of, i. 440 occurrence of. i. 499 peroxide, i. 505 preparation of, i. 500 properties of. i. 500 Hydrolysis by enzyme action, i. 282 definition of. i. 504 of fats. i. 305 of proteids, i. 292 Hydroqninone, i. 569 ll.\ men. ii. 162 11\ o-glossus muscle, ii. 126 Hyperesthesia, homolateral, after hemisection of t he cord, ii. 233 Hypennel ropia, ii. 313 range of accommodation in. ii. 314 Hyperpnoea, i. l hi from muscular activity, i. 1 12 Hypertonic solutions, physiological definition of. i. 69 Hypertonicity, definition of. i. ::7 Hypophysis cerebri, function of. i. 273 Hypotonicity, definition of. i. ,';7 Hypozanthin, i. 553 of muscles, ii. 107 relation of, to uric-acid formation, i. 338 Ii i. calorimeter, principle of, i. 504 Icterus, i. -.'l!i, .Ml [dants of the germ-plasm, ii. 5n.", Idiomuscular contraction, ii. ".'7. 92, 128 Idioplasm as a l»:i~ i~ of heredity, ii. 199 Idio-ventricular rhythm, i. 152 Ids of the germ-plasm, ii. 503 Illusions, visual, in sizes of objects, ii. 354 of space perception, ii. 351 Imbibition of water, i. 504 Immunity, inherited, ii. 498 Impregnation, ii. 466 Incus, ii. 366 Independent irritability of muscle, ii. 25 Index of refraction of the aqueous humor, ii. 303 of the [ens, ii. 303 of the vitreous humor, ii. 303 Indifferent point of polarized nerves, ii. 64 Indirect vision, color sensations in, ii. 333 Indol, i. 571 eliminat ion of. i. 340 occurrence of, in feces, i. 320 Induced currents, making and breaking shocks with, ii. 40 prevention of spread of. ii. 44 electric currents, stimulating effect of, ii. 33 Induction apparatus, schema of, ii. 33 Infections, intra-uterhie, ii. 498 Infections diseases, germinal transmission of, ii. 498 Inferior laryngeal nerve, respiratory function of, i. 464 mesenteric ganglion, reflex activity of, i. 392 oblique muscle, ii. •.'!»!! rectus muscle, ii. -.'!»!• Inflammation, emigration of leucocytes in, i. 83 Infra-hyoidei muscles, i. 405 Infundibular body, function of. i. 272 Inharmonic overtones, ii. 386 Inheritance, facts of, ii. 494 of acquired characters, ii. 496 of diseases, ii. 498 theories of, ii. 498 Inhibition from cortica, stimulation, ii. 224 in the central nervous system, ii. 224 of the heart, reflex, i. 172 Inhibitory centre, cardiac, localization of, i. 176 tonus of. i. 176 centres, respiratory, i. 457 nerves of the heart, i. 101 of the intestines, i. 385 of the pancreas, i. 233 of the spleen, i. '.','.','■', of the stomach, i. 382 Innervation of the blood-vessels, i. 192 of the heart, i. 148 Inorganic salts of the hlood, i. 50 of urine, i 34 l relation of. to blood coagulation, i. 56, 57 to irritability, ii. 59 to the heart heat. i. 151, 189 Inosil . i 57:'. Insanity, relation of brain-weight to, ii. 2/78 variations of muscular tonus in. ii. 220 Insecl muscle, time of contraction in. ii. 108 Inspiration, enlargement of the thorax in, i. 398 muscles of. i :;<•>, 101 Inspiratory centre, i 157 Intensity of visual sensations, ii. 339 Intercostales muscles, respiratory action of, i. 102, 107 Intermedins nerve of Wrisberg, central path of. ii 236 Intermittent pulse, i. Ml Internal capsule, grouping of fibres in. ii. 248 ear, anatomy of. ii. '.\'\ rectus muscle, ii. 299 secret ion. definition of. i. -.'05 of the adrenal bodies, i. 272 GENERAL INDEX. 537 Internal secretion of the kidneys, i. 274 of the liver, i. 265 of the ovaries, i. -'71 of the pancreas, i. 266 of the pituitary body, i. 273 of tlie testis, i. 273 of the thyroids, i. 270 Intestinal contents, reaction of, i. 310 digestion, i. 299 juice, i. 243 movements, i. 382-385 Intestines, innervation of, i. 3a4 intrinsic nervous mechanism of, i. 384 large, absorption in, i. 314 pendular movements of, i. 384 peristalsis of, i. 382 putrefactive changes in, i. 310 small, absorpl ion in, i. 313 vaso-motor nerves of, i. 206' Intracardiac pressure, i. lo7, 1 25, 126 methods of measuring, i. 129, 130 Intracranial pressure, relation of, to blood-pres- sure, ii. 287 Intra-ocular images, ii. 320 Intrapolar region, ii. 62 Intrapnlmonary pressure, i. 408 intrathoracic pressure, i. 397, 409 Intravascular clotting, i. 60, ' > 1 Intrinsic nerves of the heart, i. 148 Introductory contractions of a contraction series, ii. 1*13 peak of tetanus curves, ii. 124 Inversion of retinal images, ii. 305 Invertase, occurrence of, i. 308 Invertebrates, conduction in the nerves of, ii. 91 Invertine, definition of, i. 280 Involuntary muscles, rigor mortis of, ii. 162 Iodine, i. 509 Iodothyrin, properties of. i. 270 Ionic theory of solutions, i. 67 Ion-proteid compounds of muscle, ii. 168 Iris, dilator nerves of, ii. 324 direct response to light by, ii. 324 innervation of, ii. 323 movements of, in accommodation, ii. 309 rate of, ii. 325 muscles of, ii. 323 relation of, to spherical aberration, ii. 315 Iron, amount of, in haemoglobin, i. 39 excretion of, i. 530 inorganic absorption of. i. 529 nutritive history of, i. 528 occurrence of, i. 528 salts, excretion of, i. 356 nutritive value of, i. 356 synthesis of, into haemoglobin, i. 529 Irradiation in the retina, ii. 349 of medullary centres, i. 201 of nerve-impulses in the central neivous sys- tem, ii. 208 Irrigating fluids for the isolated heart, i. L89, 191 Irritability, definition of. ii. 20, 23 effect of blood-supply on. ii. 66 of constant current on, ii. 62 of repeated stimuli on, ii. •>."> of living matter, i. 18 of muscle, ii. 25 of nerve-fibres, ii. 21 of nerves, ii. 2 1 ami muscles, conditions affecting, ii. 55 effect of section on, ii. 69 of ova, ii. 22 Irritants, classification of, ii. 23 conditions determining their efficiency, ii. 28 effect of, on irritability, ii. 55 of variations in strength of, ii. 39 Irritants, relation of, to the response, ii. 24 [schsemia of heart muscle, i. l-i [schio-cavernosi muscles, ii. 449 [schio cavernosus, action of, in erection, ii. 464 Iso-butyl alcohol, i. 539 Iso-butyric acid, i. 7>:;'.i Iso-dynamic equivalence of foods, i, 365 Isolated apex of frog's heart, i. 1 — conduction in nerve-trunks, ii. 79 Isolation of the heart, i. 148, 191 Isomaltose, i. 565 isometric contractions, definition of. ii. 110 l-ii pent; 1 alcohol, i. 539 isotonic contractions, definition of. ii. llo solul ions, i. 36, on Isotonicity, i. 36, 68 Isotropic substance of muscle-fibres, ii. 104 Iso- valerianic acid. i. .">39 .1 \i n dick. i. 249, "'1 1 Jecorin, i. 564 Joints, classification of, ii. 415 .lumping, ii. 420 K.vkvokinksis, i. 20 Karyokinetic figures in mature nerve-cells, ii. 202 Katabolism, definition of, i. 19 Katelectrotonus, ii. 62 Kathodal contraction, ii. 35 Kathode, physical definition of, ii. 52 physiological definition of. ii. 52 Keratin, i. 580 Ketoses, definition of, i. 561 Keys, electric, ii. 30 Kidneys, blood-flow through, i. 255 histology of. i. 2 19 internal secretion of. i. 274 nerve-endings in. i. 251 vaso-motor nerves of, i. 207. 256 " Klopf-versuch " of Goltz, i. 175 Knee-kick, reinforcement of, ii. 222 Krause's membrane, ii. 104 Kymograph, i. 89 Labia majora, ii. 462 minora, ii. 462 Labial consonants, ii. -137 Labio-dental frictionals, ii. l">- I. allium tympanicum of tin- internal ear. ii. 377 vesl ibulare of (he limbus, ii. 377 Labor, nature of, ii l-l stages of. ii. i t; • Labor-pains, ii. 179 Labyrinth of the ear, anatomy of. ii. 371 Lactalbumin, i. 261 Lactation, ovulation during, ii. 156 Lacteal vessels, i. ."I - I .acteals, absorpl ion t hrough, i. 31 1 Lactic acid, i 545 fermental ion, i. ."> 15 occurrence of, in the stomach, i. 289 of muscles, ii. 10- Lacto globulin, i. 261 Lac; ose, i 202. 565 Laky blood, i. 35 Lamina spiralis, ii. 372 Laminae of medullar; tube in the foetus, ii. 205 Langerhans, bodies of, i. 232 I ianol in. i. '.'."'7. .">7."> Larue intestine, digestion in. i. 309 Laryngeal muscles, specific action of. \\. ij- oerve, recurrent, ii. 128 superior, ii. 128 Laryngoscope, ii. 129 I. :n vii \. carl ilages of. ii. 12.". closure of. during muscular effort, ii. 123 538 GENERAL INDEX. Larynx, muscles of, Li. 125 in i \ ea hi', 11. I'J- 31 nictate of, ii. i.l Latent ana- of the cortex, ii. 21n the contraction curve, ii 111 Local signs of sensations, ii. 394 Localization, cutaneous variations of, ii. 395 in the skin, theory of, ii. 395 of cell-groups in the cerebral cortex, ii. 241 of cortical cell-groups for afferent impulses, ii. 252 of pain sensations, ii 399 of touch sensat ion-, ii. 394 power, relation of. to mobility, ii. 394 Locomotion, ii. 420 Locomotor ataxy, disturbance of equilibrium in, ii. 105 mechanisms, action of. ii. Ill I. new- hypothesis of the structure of proto- plasm, i. •_':; Long-reed register of the voice, ii.432 Long tracts of the cord, terminations of, ii. 235 Loop of Henle, i. 250 Loudness of the voice, factors determining, ii. 430 physical cause of, ii. 381 Luminiferous ether, rates of vibration of, Li. 331 Luminosity, relative, of spectral colors, ii. 340 Luminous sensations, intensity of, ii. 339 Lungs, capacity of, i. 4'.'7 ini'Ve-supply of. i. 165 structure of. i. 396 vaso-motor nerves of, i. 205 Lunulas of the semilunar valves, i. Ill Lustre in \isual sensations, explanation of, ii. Lutein, i. 571 Luxus consumption, i. 348 Lymph, i. 33 amount of. i. 1 hi definition of. i. to formation of. i. 71 gases of, i. mi glands, i. 140'. GENERAL IXDEX. 539 Lymph, mechanical theory of the origin of, i. 75 movement of, i. 71, 146 pressure of, i. 146 secretion of, i. 21 1 Lymphagogues, action of, i ?•!. 7i Lymphatic system, nature of, i. 143 Lymphatics of the heart, i. 186 Lymphocytes, i. 48 Lysatin, i. 55i Lysatinin, relation of, to una formation, i. 337, 551 Lysin, i. 552 Macroceph \i.i< brains, weight of, ii. 275 .Macula acustica, ii. 373 lutea, ii. 327 Macula? acustica. relation of, to state of equi- librium, ii. 1<>7 Magnesium carbonate, i. 527 nutritive history of, i. 527 occurrence of, i. 527 phosphates, i. 527 Making contraction, point of origin of, ii. 35 " Slaking "' shock, ii. 31 Male births, relative number of, ii. 483 pronucleus, ii. 466 Males, rate of growth in, ii. 488 Malic acid, i. 558 Malleus, ii. 365 ligaments of, ii. 366 Malpighian corpuscle of the kidney, structure of, i. 249 Maltase, i. 280, 565 in starch digestion, i. 285 occurrence of, i. 308 Mammary glands, ii. 443, 462 histological changes in, i. 262 in pregnancy, ii. 477 normal secretion of, i. 264 secretory nerves of, i. 263 structure of, i. 261 Manuose, i. •"><>.' Manometer, differential, i. 131 elastic, i. 127 maximum, i. 107 mercurial, i. -7 Manubrium of the malleus, ii. 365 Marsh gas. i. 532 Masseter muscle, i. 372 Mastication, i. 372 Masticatory movements, effect of, on taste-sen- sations, ii. Ill Mastoid antrum, ii. 363 " Mastzellen," relation of, to colostrum corpus- cles, i. 263 Maturation of germ-cells, significance of, ii. 454 of nerve-cells, ii. 177 of spermatozoa, ii. 445 of tiie ovum. ii. I'll Meat extracts, physiological action of. i. 359 Meats, composii ion of, i. 278 Meatus auditorius interims, ii. 373 Mechanical stimulation of uerves, ii. 25, 56 strain, influence of, on neuroblasts, ii. i?i> work (if muscular contraction, ii. 138 Meconium, biliary salt- in. i. •" I 1 Medial lemniscus, ii. 226 Medullary -heath, development of, in the C6E tral nervous system, ii. 181 in the peripheral nerves, ii. 180 significance of, ii. L80 tube, fetal, ii. 204 lamina' of, in t lie foetus, ii. 205 Medullation, central, progressive character of, ii. 1-1 of nerve-fibres, significance of. ii. 283 peripheral, ii. 180 Medusae, rate of conduction in. ii. 89 staircase contractions in. ii. 112 Melauius, i. 57 I Melicyl alcohol, i. 540 Membrana basilaris, ii. 374 flaccida, ii. 365 granulosa of the Graafian follicle, ii. 454 reticulata, ii. .17- tectoria. ii. 377. :!7i» tympani, ii. '■'>*> I Membrane of Reissner, ii. 371. :;7!» Membranous labyrinth of the ear, ii. 372 Menopause, ii. 159, 190 Menstrual ion. ii. 457 age of onset of. ii. 459 cessation of, at the climacteric, ii. 490 genera] disturbances accompanying, ii. 459 in animals, ii. 460 relation of ovulation to. ii. 456 theory of, ii. 460 Mental activity, relation of cerebral circulation to, ii. 288 Menthol, action of, on cold nerves, ii. 398 Mercapturic acids, i. 517 Mercury manometer, description of. i. -7 Metabolism, conditions influencing, i. 359 definition of, i. 20 during sleep, i, 361 starvation, i. 3(52 effect of temperature on, i. 362 influence of the cell-nucleus on, i. 22 intensity of. in the brain, ii. 288 methods of estimating, i. 343 Metaphosphoric acid. i. 514 Methsemoglobin, i. 1 1. 573 Methane, origin of, i. 532 Methods, physiological, i. 31 Methyl amido-acetic acid, i. 538 mercaptan, i. 534 seleuide, i. 534 telluride, i. 5:>4 violet, in testing for mineral acid-, i. 289 Methylamine, i. 541 Meynert's commissure, ii. 238 M icellsa, definition of, i. 25 Microcephalic brains, weight of, ii. '.'75 Microcephalics, ii. 268 Micturition, i. :;-:i en i,-e for. i. 391, 393 cerebral control of. ii. 070 nervous mechanism of. i. 392 reflex character of, ii. 213 Mi. Idle ear, ii. 3d-.' inflammatory disease of, ii. 364 mechanism of, ii. 368 muscles of. ii. .".ii!" Migration of neuroblasts, ii. 17ii Milk, composition of, i. 261 mineral constituents of. i. 530 normal secret ion of. i. 26 1 Milk-sugar, i. 565 Millon's reaction for proteids, i. 576 nature of, i. .".flit wit h phenol, i. 569 M ineral acids, tests for. i. 289 constituents, amount of, in the tissues, i. 630 Mitosis, i. 20 Modiolus, ii. 372 M..i,cular weight, relation of. to physiological act ion. ii. 60 Molecules, physical ami physiological, i. 25 Monochromal ic light, ii. 316 M .nuclear leucocytes, i. |s M.ms Veneris, ii. 162 Monstrosities, congenital, ii. 1**1 origin of, ii. 183 Morgagni, v.m ricles of. ii. 122 540 « / i:XERA L INDEX. Morphin, effect of, on body-temperature, i. 472 Morula, ii. 47o Motor areas, cortical serial arrangement of, ii. 247 degeneration after removal of, ii. '-44 paralysis following removal of, ii. 269 physiological characters of, ii. 243 subdivision of, into centres, ii. 247 centres, degree of separateness of, ii. 248 of the human cortex, ii. 250 disturbance from hemisection of the cord, ii. 230 end-plates, latent period of, ii. 103 transmission of excitation bv means of, ii. 82 nerves, fatigue of, ii. 96 rate of conduction in, ii. 89 Mouth, temperature in, i. 469 Movements of joints, varieties of, ii. 416 of spermatozoa, ii. 444 of the eyeball, ii. 298 Mucin of bile, i. 325 of gastric juice, i. 288 of saliva, i. 283 physiological value of, i. 221 properties of, i. 578 , secretion of, i. "217 Mucous glands, histology of, i. 216 Miiller's experiment, i. 452 Multiple conceptions, ii. 482 control from the cortex, ii. 250 Murexid, i. 555 Muscae volitantes, ii. 320 Muscarin, i. 543 action of, on the heart, i. 150 Muscle, accelerator urinae, ii. 449 aryteno-epiglott ideal), ii. 426 arytenoid, ii. 426 bulbo-cavernosus, ii. 4 19 chemistry of, ii. 159 ciliary, in accommodation, ii. 309 crico-arytenoid, lateral, ii. 426 crico-thyroid, ii. 426 currents of action in, ii. 150 of rest in, ii. 147 digastric, i. -".72: ii. 426 elasticity of, ii. 105 erector clitoridis. ii. 464 external rectus, ii. 299 fatigue of, ii. 70 frog's, rate of conduction in, ii. 89 gases of. ii. 168 general physiology of, ii. 17 genio-hyoid, i. 372; ii. 126 glycogenic function of, i. 330 human, rate of conduction in, ii. 89 hyo-glossus, ii. 426 independent irritability of, ii. 25 inferior oblique, ii. 299 rectus, ii. 299 inorganic constituents of, ii. 168 internal rectus, ii. 299 in voluntary, properties of, i. 370 limitation of the rate of st i initiation in, ii. 126 masseter, i. 327 mineral constituents of, i. 530 inylodiyoid, i. 372 : ii. 126 nitrogenous extractives of, ii. 166 DOn-nitrogenous constituents of, ii. 168 obliquus externus, i. 407 interims i. |u7 omo-hyoid, ii. 125 posterior crico-arytenoid, ii. 426 pterygoid, external, i. 372 internal, i. 372 pyramidalis, i. H>7 reaction of, ii. L59 Muscle, red, capacity for tetanic contraction of, ii. 127 retractor lentis. of fishes, ii. 306 skeletal, sensory nerve-endings in, ii. 402 specific gravity of, ii. 159 stapedius, ii. 370 sterno-hyoid, ii. 425 sterno-thyroid, ii. 425 striated, histology of, ii. 104 optical properties of, ii. 103 Btylo-hyoid, ii. 426 superior oblique, ii. 299 rectus, ii. 299 temporalis, i. 372 thy ro-arytenoid. ii. 426 external, ii. 121 internal, ii. 424 thy lo-hyoid, ii. 425 transversalis abdominis, i. 407 trapezius, i. 405 Muscle-contraction, Engelmaun's theory of, ii. 105 Muscle-plasma, ii. 161, 163 Muscle-proteids, precipitation temperature of, ii. 166 Muscles, abdomiuales. action of, in vomiting, i. 387 respiratory function of, i. 407 absolute force of, ii. 141 action of, upon the bones, ii. 417 classification of, ii. 18 degeneration of, after section of motor nerves. ii. 48, 54. 70 endurance of. ii. 76 erectores penis, ii. 449 spinse, i. 405 expiratory, i. 407 glycogen of. i. 33i i human, fatigue of, with artificial stimulation, ii. 134 infrahyoidei, i. 405 inspiratory, i. 399, 405 intercostal, i. 402, 407 ischio-cavernosi, ii. 449 levatores ani, i. 407 costarum, i. 102 of mastication, i. 372 of the eye, ii. 299 of the iris, ii. 323 of the middle ear. ii. 369 pectorales, i. 405 quadrati lumborum, i. 399 rate of conduct ion in, ii. 89 rhomboidei, i. 405 scaleni, i. 401 serrati postici. i. 399, 402 sterno-cleido-mastoid, i. 404 tensor tympani. ii. 369 thermogenic function of. i. 490 triangulares sterni, i. H»7 vaso-motor aerves of, i. 210 Muscle-serum, ii. L66 Muscle-sounds, ii. 132 Muscle-spindles, ii. 102 Muscle-structure, influence of, on its contrac- tion, ii. 1H7 Muscle-tonus, ii. 1 13 Muscular contracl ions, effect of drugs on, ii. 137 Of Support on the height of, L22 of temporal u re on, ii. 136 graphic record of, ii. 99 influences affecting, ii. 107 post-mortem, ii. 160 BOB rce of energy in, ii. 74 effort, closure of larynx in, ii. 423 exercise, effecl of, on metabolism, i. 359 on the pulse-rate, i. 121 a EXE HAL IXDEX. 541 Muscular exercise, effect of, on the rate of respiration, i. 426 on the respiratory exchanges, i. 433 quotient, i. 138 on the sweat glands, i. 260 on the venous circulation, i. 95 inhibition from cortical stimulation, ii. 224 movements, relations of antagonistic muscles in, ii. 418 sensatious, cortical area for, ii. 253 definition of, ii. 390 effect of hemisection of the cord on, ii. 235 in estimation of weights, ii. 403 nature of, ii. 401 path of conduction for, in the cord, ii. 235 psychological value of; ii. '■'>'. il work, effect of stimulants on, ii. 75 Musical sounds, characteristics of, ii. 387 tones, beats of, ii. 386 limits in the pitch of, ii. 282 production of, ii. 381 Mycoderma aceti, i. 537 Mydriatics, ii. 325 Mylo-hyoid muscle, i. 372 ; ii. 426 Myo-albumin, ii. 166 Myo-albumose, ii. 166 Myogen-fibrin, ii. 164 Myoglobulin, ii. 166 Myogonic theory of the causation of the heart- beat, i. 150 Myogram, ii. 34 definition of, ii. 100 effect of temperature on the form of, ii. 136. Myograph, ii. 35 description of, ii. 100 double, of Hering, ii. 36 Myohsematin, i. 578; ii. 166 Myopia, ii. 313 range of accommodation in, ii. 314 Myosin, ii. 163 absorption of, i. 315 ferment, ii. 161, 163 Myosin-fibrin, ii. 164 Myosinogen, ii. 163 temperature of heat coagulation of, ii. 165 Myotics, ii. 325 Myxcedema, i. 269 Native albumins, i. 577 Nausea from disturbance of equilibrium, ii. 405 Near-point of vision, ii. 312 Negative after-images, ii. 346 pressure in the auricles, i. 137 in the heart, i. 98 in the thorax, i. 05 in the veins, i. !H variation in muscles, rate of propagation, ii. 152 relation of, to the contraction, ii. 153 of muscle-currents, ii. 150 of nerve-currents, ii. 154 of the beating heart, i. 153 Nerve, aurieulo-teniporal, i.'.'I- chorda tympani. i. 101, 210 gustatory function of, ii. 1 10 coronary, of t be tortoise, i. 164 depressor, i. 172, 203 facial, secretory fibres of. i. 219 general physiology of. ii. 17 glossopharyngeal, gustatory function of, ii. 410 secretory fibres of. i. 218 Jacobson's, i. 218 lingual, secretory fibres of, i. 219 oculomotor, ii. 323 recurrent laryngeal, ii. 128 small superficial petrosal, i. 218 superior laryngeal, ii. 428 Nerve, vagus, cardiac branches of, i. 159 gastric branches of. i. 381 intestinal branches of, i. 385 pulmonary branches of, i. 465 repiratory functions of. i. i59 secretory fibres of. i. 232, 239 trophic influence of, on the heart, i. 166 Nerve-cells, atrophy of, from disuse, ii. 195 changes in, with age, ii. 400 chemical changes in. ii. 191 classification of, ii. 177 connection between, ii. 206 degeneration <>f the cell-bodies of, ii. 199 diaxonic, ii. ITS effect of exercise on, ii. 76 fatigue of, ii. 191 generation of impulses in, ii. 188 growth of, ii. 176 histological change in, due to functional activity, ii. 192 human, size of, ii. 171 internal structure of, ii. 179 maturation of, ii. 177 morphology of. ii. 17^1 number of, in the central nervous system, ii. 283 nutrition of, ii. 190 nutritive control of, over nerve-fibres, ii. 198 of animals, size of, ii. 175 of spinal ganglia, development of, ii. 178 peculiarities of, ii. 174 pyramidal, ii. 178 rate of discharge from, ii. 189 regeneration of, ii. 201 relation of size and function in, ii. 175 senescence of, ii. 182, 490 significance of the branches of, ii. 186 summation of stimuli in. ii. 190 volume relation of, ii. 175 Nerve-elements, primitive segmental arrange- ment of, ii. 205 Nerve-endings in the liver, i. 245 in the salivary glands, i. 220 in the skin, ii. 392 Nerve-fibres, reaction of, ii. 170 classification of, ii. 21 cortical, increase in the number of, during growth, ii. 282 fatigue in, ii. 95 functions of, ii. 21 Nerve-impulse, definition of. ii. 25 direction of the passage of. ii. 184 electrical variation accompanying, ii. 183 genera! ion of, ii. 187 in peripheral nerves, ii. 1>.'I peripheral diffusion of. ii. '.'is reversed, in spinal ganglion-cells, ii. 185 theories of. ii. 07 transmission of. from neurone to neurone, ii. 207 Nerve-muscle preparation of a frog, ii. 34 Nerves, action cu rrents in, ii. 153 auditory, central path of. ii. 237 augmentor, of i lie heart, i. ltiT cardiac, i. 1 1* cervical sympai hel ic, i. 193 chemist ry of. ii. 169 cross sin uring of, ii. 200 current of resl in. ii. 1 10 degeneration of, after section, ii, 69, 78 depressor, of the heart, i. 172 fatigue of, ii. 75 glossopharyngeal, central conduction paths for. ii. 236 in man. stimulation of. ii. 51 law of stimulation of. with galvanic current, ii. 50 542 GENERAL INDEX. Nerves, limitation of the rate of stimulation in, ii, 126 lingual, gustatory function of, ii. 410 medullation of. ii. 180 non-mcdullated, rate of conduction in. ii. 90 of common sensation, central conduction paths of, ii. 230 of cutaneous sensations, central conduction paths of, ii. 233 of dermal sensation, una of distribution of, ii. 23] of invertebrates, rate of conduction in, ii. 91 of taste, nuclei of origin of, ii. 236 of temperature, ii. -'197 of tile bile \e-scls. i. 248 of Wrisberg intermedins), central path of, ii. 236 olfactory, central paths of, ii. 241 o[itic, central paths of, ii. 238 phrenic, i. 463 rate of conduction in. ii. B9 secondary degeneration of. ii. 197 septal, of the frog's heart, i. 166 specific energy of, ii. 232 splanchnic, i. 1?:; trigeminal, i. 463 centra] paths of, ii. 238 vagus, course of the afferenl fibres in, ii. 236 Nerve-trunk-, isolated conduction in, ii. 79 Nervi erigentes, ii. 464 intestinal branches of, i. 385 Neukomm's test for bile acids, i. 545 Neuridin, i. 5 13 Neurin, i. 5 13 Neuroblast, development of, ii. 176 Neurogenic theory of the causation of the heart-beat', i. 149 Neuro-keratin, i. 580 of nerves, ii. 169 Neuromuscular spindle, ii. 390 Neurone, definition of, ii. 17;; Neurones, ii. 21 afferent, to the spinal cord, ii. 203 changes in number and size of, ii. 280 conduct ion in, ii. 97 connection by, ii. 206 double conduction in, ii. 185 increase in number of, during growth, ii. 282 internal structure of, ii. 179 polarity of, ii. 184 total number of, ii. 2S3 Neutral salts, effects of, on blood coagulation, i. 62 Neutrophils, i. 47 Nicotin, action of, on end-plates, ii. 27 on intestinal movements, i. 384 on secretory nerves, i. 229 on sympathetic ganglia, ii. 219 Nissl method for nerve-cells, ii. 195 substance, iron in. ii. 191 of nerve-cells, ii. 179 Nitric oxide, i. 512 hemoglobin, i. 39. 512 Nitrogen equilibrium, definition of, i. 344. 512 free, of muscles, ii. 168 history of. in the body. i. 512 inhalation, i. 440 occurrence of, i. 510 of the feces, i. ::■-'" preparation of. i. 510 tension of the hi I. i. 117 Nitrogenous equilibrium, definition of, i. 344. 512 excreta of milk, i. 262 of sweat, i. 259 extractives of muscle, ii. 166 of the spleen, i. 333 Nitrogenous metabolism, estimation of, i. 343 Nitrous oxide, inhalation of, i. 440 properties of, i. 512 Nodal point in the simplest dioptric system, ii. 301 Nceud vital, i. 456; ii. 236 Noises, definition of, ii. 388 Non-medullated nerves, rate of conduction in, ii. 90 stimulation fatigue of, ii. 180 Non-polarizable electrodes, ii. 29 Nose, anatomy of, ii. 408 respiratory tract of, ii. 408 Nucleic acid, i. 579 Nuclein bases, i. 552 composition, i. 556, 579 Nucleo-histon of the blood-plates, i. 49 relation of, to intravascular clotting, i. 61 Nucleo-proteids, classification of, i. 577 properties of, i. 579 Nucleus, functions of, i. 22 relation of, to oxidation, i. 503 Nutrition of living matter, i. 18 of nerve-cells, ii. L90 of the embryo, ii. 475 Nutritive control of nerve-cell bodies over nerve-fibres, ii. \\\> value of albuminoids, i. 349 of carbohydrates, i, 353 of fats, i. 350 of proteids, i. 276, 345 of salts, i. 354 of water, i. 354 Nymphse, ii. 462 Nystagmus after cerebellar injury, ii. 272 Obliquus externus. respiratory action of, i. 407 interims, respiratory action of, i. 407 Occlusion of the bile-duct, effect of, i. 249 Oculomotor nerve, ciliary fibres of, ii. 311 relation of, to the iris, ii. 323 Odors, ii. 410 CEderna, i. 148 of the glottis, ii. 422 Oesophagus, deglutition in the, i. 374 Oils, effect of, on gastric secretion, i. 241 on pancreatic secretion, i. 236 Old age of the central nervous system, ii. 295 (defines, i. 542 Oleic acid, i. 541-560 Olfactory area of the cortex, ii. 253 cells, ii. 408 epithelium, ii. 408 nerves, central paths of. ii. 241 paths lot he brain, ii. 109 stimuli, conditions affecting, ii. 409 tracts, section of, in sharks, ii. 264 Omo-hyoid muscle, ii. 425 Oncometer, i. 255 Ontogenetic development of nerve-cells, ii. 177 Onychodromus, reproduction of, ii. 442 ( >ocyte. ii. 451 Oophorin tablets, action of, i. 274 Opening of the chest, effect of, on heart, i. 115 ( > | >l 1 1 halmometer, ii. 304 I ipht halmoscope, ii. 326 Opium, effect of, on respiratory rhythm, i. 425 optic commissure, decussation of optic fibres in, ii. 238 nerve, currents of action in. ii 154 nerve-fibres, number of, ii. 330 nerves, central paths of. ii. 238 i-ort ical centres of, ii. 210 efferent fibres of, ii. 240 t halami, functions of, ii. 271 Optical constants of the eye, ii. 303 (iEXEBAL IX I) EX. 543 Optical illusions in binocular vision, ii. 359 of space perceptions, ii. 351 properties of striated muscle, ii. 103 Optograms, ii. 330 Organ of Corti, structure of, ii. 377 of Golgi in tendons, ii. 402 " Organeiweiss," i. 346 Organization in the central nervous system, ii. 285 relation of educability to the establishment of, ii. 286 Organs, growth of, ii. 486 Ornithin. i. 552 Orthophosphoric acid, i. 514 <)s orbiculare of the incus, ii. 366 Osazones of glycoses, i. 562 Oscillatory activity of the retina, ii. 344 Osmosis, definition of, i. 65 relation of, to secretion, i. 213 Osmotic pressure, definition of. i. 65 method of determining, i. 67. 68 relation of, to concentration, i. 66 Osones, preparation of. i. 562 Ossicles, auditory, ii. 365 of the ear, movements of, ii. 367 Osteomalacia, i. 524, 525 ovariotomy in. i. 274 Osteoporosis, i. 525 Otitis media, ii. 364 Otoconia, ii. 374 Otoliths, ii. 374 Ova. ii. 440 number of, in human ovary, ii. 451 Ovaries, ii. 443 effect of removal of, on menstruation, ii. 459 internal secretion of, i. 274 Ovariotomy, effects of, i. 274 Ovary, structure of, ii. 454 Overtones, definition of. ii. 383 inharmonic, ii. 3S6 Oviducts, ii. 443. See Fallopian tubes. Ovulation, ii. 455 Ovum, chemistry of, ii. 450 fertilization of, ii. 466 human, structure of. ii. 449 maturation of, ii. 451 physiological properties of. ii. 22 segmentation of, ii. 467 Stages in the maturation of, ii. 452 Oxalate solutions, effect of, on blood coagula- tion, i. 63 Oxalic acid, i. 557 Oxalnrie acid, i. 555 Oxidases, i. 281 Oxidation, i. 501 physiological, Hoppe-Seyler's theory of, i. 505 Traube's theory of, i. 502 Oxidizing enzymes, i. 280 Oxybutyric acid, i. 548 Oxycholin, i. 543 Oxygen, alveolar tension of, i. 413 occurrence of, i. 500 prepara' ion of, i. 501 properties of, i. 501 storage of, in muscle, ii. L69 supply, relation of, to irritability, ii. 68 tension in the blood, i. 415 respiratory effects of varying, i. I In Oxygen-absorption, coefficient of, i. 115 conditions affect ing, i. 129 cutaneous, i. 122 estimation of, i. 428 ( Oxygen-dyspnoea, i. Ill Oxyhemoglobin, composition of, i. 38 dissociation of, i. 415, 501 Oxyntic cells of gastric glands, i. 237 Oxyphenyl-acetic acid, i. 570 Oxyphenyl-amido-propionic acid, i. 570 Oxyphiles, i. 17 Ozone inhalation, i. 140 preparation of, i. 502 properties of, i. 502 Pacinian body, ii. 391 of the penis, ii. 1 1!' Pain nerves, evidence for t he existence of, ii. 232 points of the skin, ii. 400 sensations of, ii. 399 Pains, transferred or sympathetic, ii. 400 Pale striped muscle, physiological peculiarities of, ii. 109 Palmitic acid, i. 541 Pancreas, anatomy of, i. 231 extirpation of. i. 266 grafting of, i. 267 histology of, i. 231 innervation of, i. 232 internal secretion of, i. 266 mineral constituents of, i. 530 secretory changes in. i. 233 vaso-motor nerves of, i. 207 Pancreatic diabetes, i. 267, 353, 563 fistulse, preparation of, i. 300 juice, amylolytic action of, i. 305 artificial, i. 301 collection of, i. 300 composition of, i. 232, 299 fat-splitting power of, i. 305 secretion, composition of, i. 232, 299 histological changes during, i. 233 nervous mechanism of, i. 232 normal mechanism of, i. 235 reflex character of, i. 236 relation of, to the character of the food, i. 237 Pangenesis, Darwin's theory of, ii. 501 Papain, i. 280 Papilla foliata of rabbits, ii. 410 Papillary muscles, i. 110 Parabamic acid. i. 555 Paracasein, i. 296 Paradoxical contraction, ii. 157 Paraffins, i. 531 Paraformic aldehyde, i. 533 Paraglobulin, amount of, in the blood, i. 53 composition of, i. 53 functions of, i. 53 origin of, i. 53 properties of, i. 53 Parallax, use of, in estimation of distance, ii. 356 Paralysis after removal of motor anas, ii. 26!' agitans, ii. 296 Brown-Sequard's, ii. 233 homolateral, after heinisection of the cord, ii. 233 Paralytic secretion, i. 229 Paramcecium, reproduction in, ii. 440 Paramyosinogen, ii. 163 temperature of heal coagulation of, ii. L65 Parapeptone, definition of, i. 292 Paraniichin. i. 579 Parathyroids, anatomy of, i. 268 fund ion of, i. 269 Paresis following removal of the cerebellum, ii. 272 from injury to motor areas, ii. '.'('.'.i Parotid gland, anatomy of, i 2Vi in nerval ion of, i. 218 Partial tones, definition of, ii. 383 Parturition, ii. 17!» Spinal cent re of, ii 1~ ' Pate* de foie gras, i. 560 Paths of conduction in the cord, clinical evi- dence on, ii. 23 1 544 GENERAL INDEX. Pause, compensatory, of the heart, i. 156 Pauses, respiratory, i. 424 Pectoral muscles, respiratory action of, i. 405 Pendular movements of the intestines, i. 384 vibrations, ii. '-'>-\ Penis, ii. 443 structure of, ii. 148 Pentamethylene-diamin, i. 543 Pentoses, i. 562 Pepsin, i. 237, 238 effect of, on blood coagulation, i. 63 preparal ion of, i. 291 properties of, i. 290 Pepsin-hydrochloric acid, action of, i. 292 Pepsinogen granules, i. 242 Peptic digestion, i. 292, 294 Peptones, absorption of, in the stomach, i. 313 definition of, i. 292, 295 effect of, on blood coagulation, i. t>2 propert ies of, i. 294, 577 Pepton-injection, effect of, on lymph format inn i. 73 toxicity of, i. 316 Perfusion cannula, i. 1.^7 Perilymph, ii. :;72 Periodic reflexes, ii. 2ln' Peripheral nerves, medullation of, ii. 180 reference of special sensations, ii. 400 reflex cenl res, i. L78 Peristalsis, definition of, i. 372 intestinal, i. 382 of the stomach, i. 37!» of the ureters, i. 389 Permeability of the capillary walls, i. 70 Peroxide of bydrogen, i. 505 Pettenkofer's reaction for bile acids, i. 324, 514 Pexinogen granules, i. 242 Pfliiger's hypothesis of the structure of proto- plasm, i. 23 law of contract ion, ii. 50 Phagocytosis, i. 48 Phakoscope, ii. 308 Phalangar process of the rods of Corti, ii. 378 Pharynx, deglutition in, i. 373 I'lieiiaceturic acid, i. 569 Phenol, i. 569 elimination of. i. 340 Phenyl-acetic acid. i. 569 Phloridzin diabetes, i. 563 Phosphates, i. -"1 I Phosphene, pressure, ii. 305, 331 Phosphoric acid, -alts of. i. 511 Phosphorus, nutritive history of. i. 515 occurrence of, i. 513 peroxide, i. 51 1 poisoning, i. 513 preparal ion of, i. 513 properties of. i. 513 Photomet ry, ii. '■'> 15 Phrenic' nerve, current of action in. ii. 151 mrves. i. 463 Phylogenetic development of nerve-cells, ii. 177 Physical molecules, di Sniti f. i. 25 Physiological anodes, definition of, ii. 52 division of labor, i. 22 molecules, i. •.'."I kat bode, definil ion of, ii. 52 abservations on afferent paths in the cord, ii. 229 rheoscope, ii. 1 K 151 -alt solut ion, ii. 59 in t ransfusions, i. 64 Physiology, definition of. i. 17 human, definil ion of. i. .'Ill met hod- employed in. i. 30 subdivisions of, i. 17, 29 Physostigmin, action of, on accommodation, ii. :;il on the eye, ii. 325 l'ia mater, weight of, ii. 274 Pigment epithelium, retinal, movements of, ii. :;:;n retinal, relation of, to adaptation of the eye, ii. 326 Pigments, biliary, i. 45, 215, 322, 530, 574 blood-, i. 37, 4-1, 573 Pilocarpin, action of, on salivary glands, i. 229 on sweat-glands, i. 260 Pilomotor mechanism, relation of, to thermo- lysis, i. 494 Pince myographique, ii. ~7 Pineal gland, calcification of, in old age, ii. 491 Pinna of the ear, ii. 362 Pitch, limits of perception of, ii. 382 of musical tones, ii. 381 of the voice, ii. 130, 432 Pituitary body, anatomy of, i. 272 functions of, i. '.'7;; internal secretion of, i. 273 extracts, action of. i. 272 membrane, ii. 408 Pivot-joint, ii. 417 Placenta, ii. 17 I Placental transmission of infectious diseases, ii. villi, ii. 474 Plain muscle, histology of, i. 369 physiology of, i. 370 tone of, i. 371 Plant-cells, assimilation in, i. 18 Plants, regeneration of lost parts in. ii. 496 Plasma of blood, i. 33, 50 oxygen absorption-coefficient of, i. 416 Plastic food-stuffs, definition of, i. 346 Plethysmograph, i. !!)(> Pneumatic cabinet, i. 453 Pneumogastric nerve. See I'/tyus. pulmonary branches of, i. 465 respiratory function of, i. 459, 460 Pneumograph, i. 423 Pohl's mercury commutator, ii. 36 Poikilotherinoiis animals, i. 4(i7 Polar amphiaster of the ripening egg, ii. 453 bodies, ii. -151 of the ovum, ii. 453 Polarity of neurones, ii. 184 Polarization, after-effects of, ii. 65 physiological, of neuroblasts, ii. 176 Polarizing current, effect of, on conductivity, ii. 50 on muscles, ii. til on nerves, ii. t>2 Pole-changers, ii. 3*i Polynucleated leucocytes, i. 48 Polypncea, i. 4-11 Polyspermy, ii. 17 1 Pomum Adami, ii. 125 Portal vein, vaso-motor nerves of, i. 209 Positive after-images, ii. •'!!<> variation of the heart during vagus stimula- tion, i. 164 Posterior association centre, ii. '.'57 Post-ganglion ic fibres of the sympathetic sys- tem, ii. 219 Post-mortem rise of temperature, i. 497 Posture sense, ii. 399 Potassium carbonates, nutritive history of, i. 520 chloride, nutritive history of, i. 519 cyanide, i. 542 occurrence of, i, 519 phosphates, nutritive history of, i. 520 relation of. to heart muscle, i. 15] salts, action of, on muscles, ii. 138 GENERAL INDEX. 545 Potassium salts, relation of, to irritability, ii. 59 toxicity of, i. 520 sulphocyanide, detection of, i. 284 occurrence of, i. 283, 542 of the urine, i. 507 thiocyanide, i. 542 Potential energy of food, i. 364 Preformation theory of heredity, ii. 500 Pre-ganglionic fibres of the sympathetic system, ii. 219 Pregnancy, effects of, on the mother, ii. 477 Presbyopia, ii. 314 Pressor nerves, i. 202 Pressure, ett'ect of, on irritability of nerves, ii. 56 influence of, on conductivity, ii. 92 intracardiac, i. 107 intrathoracic, i. 396, 409 intraventricular, i. 125 of the lymph, i. 146 Pressure-points of the skin, ii. 396 Pressure-sensations, fusion of, ii. 394 Pressure-sense, delicacy of, ii. 392 of the tympanic membrane, ii. 382 Primary position of the eye, ii. 299 taste-sensations, ii. 412 Principal foci in a dioptric system, ii. 302 point of the simplest dioptric system, ii. 301 ray in the simplest dioptric system, ii. 301 Processus brevis of the malleus, ii. 365 gracilis sive folianus of the malleus, ii. 365 Projection system of fibres, origin of, from central cells, ii. 205 Pronucleus, female, ii. 453 male, ii. 466 Propeptones, definition of, i. 292 Propionic acid, i. 538 Proptosis after cerebellar injury, ii. 272 Propyl alcohol, i. 536, 538 Prostate glands, ii. 443 histology of, ii. 448 secretion of, ii. 446 Prostatic fluid, ii. 446, 448 Protagon, i. 559 of medullary substance, ii. 170 Protamin, nature and origin of, i. 24 Protamins, properties of, i. 580 Proteid, affinity of cell-substance for, i. 568 circulating, definition of, i. 346 metabolism during starvation, i. 363 effect of muscular work on, i. 360 end-products of, i. 337 Proteid-absorption, mechanism of, i. 316 Proteids, absorption of, i. 315 classification of, i. 576 color reactions of, i. 576 combined, classification of, i. 579 combustion equivalent of, i. 365 diffusion of, i. 70 dynamic value of. i. 175 effect of, on glycogen formation, i. 328 gastric digestion of, i. 292 general reactions of, i. 575 significance of, i. 2 1 living, theoretical structure of, i. 23, 24 molecular structure of, i. 581 nutritive- value of, i. 276, 345 of milk, i. 261 of muscle, precipitation temperatures of, ii. 166 of muscle-serum, ii. 166 of the blood, i 49, 50 origin of fat from, i. 351 osmotic pressure of, i. 69 putrefaction of, in the intestines, i. 310 rapidity of oxidation of, i. 347 relation of, to muscular work, ii. 74 Proteids, simple, classification of, i. 576 substitutes for, in the diet, i. 348 synthesis of, i. 518, 582 tryptic digestion of, i. 303 vegetable, i. 577 Proteolysis, i. 293 tryptic, i. 303 value of, i. 315 Proteolytic enzymes, definition of, i. 280 Proteose injection, effects of, i. 316 Proteoses, definition of, i. 292 properties of, i. 577 Prothrombin, i. 58 Protoplasm, i. 17, 199 Pseudo-mucoid, i. 578 Pseudoscopic vision, ii. 318, 357 Psychical powers of the spinal cord, ii. 215 Psycho-physic law, ii. 340 of Fechner, ii. 393 Pterygoid muscles, i. 372 Ptomaines, chemical structure of, i. 542 Ptyalin, i. 221, 280 action of, i. 284, 286, 566 occurrence of, i. 284 Puberty, ii. 489 Pulmonary circulation, i. 78, 103 innervation of, i. 205 ventilation, forces concerned in, i. 413 Pulse, arterial, cause of, i. 93 celerity of, i. 1 12 definition of, i. 139 dicrotic wave of, i. 143 extinction of, i. 94 frequency of, i. 121, 141 regularity of, i. 141 respiratory variations in the rate of, i. 451 size of, i. 1 11 tension of, i. 141 transmission of, i. 140 relation of, to body-temperature, i. 471 respiratory, i. 96 Pulse-curve, i. 142 Pulse-rate, diurnal variations of, 121 Pulse-volume of the heart, definition of. i. 105 Pupil, changes during accommodation in, ii. 311 in size of, ii. 323 condition of, in sleep, ii. 325 dilator nerves, ii. 324 size of, in old age, ii. 314 Pupillary reflex to light, ii. 323 I'lirin, i. 553 bases, i. 552 in leucocythsemia, i. 557 Purkinje-Sanson's images, ii. 307 Purkinje's figure, ii. 321 phenomenon, ii. 340 explanat ion of. ii. .'! 12 Purposeful reflexes, ii. 215 Putrefaction, intestinal, products of, i. 310 Putrescin, i. 543 Pyin, i. 579 Pyramidal fibres, number of, ii. 216 nerve-cells, development of, ii. 178 tracts in t he cold. ii. 215 geminal fibres of, ii. 215 size of, ii. 252 Pyramidal is muscle, expiratory action of, i. 107 Pyridin, L 571 Pyrocatechin, i. 569 Qi ldbat] lumborum, respiratory action of, i. 399 Quadruplets, ii. 183 Qualitv of musical tones, ii. 383 of the voice, ii. 130 546 <; i;m:l'.\l index. Quinine, action of, on coagulation of muscle- plasma, ii. 164 hydrochlorate, actii f, on salivary glands, i. 222 Quintuplets, ii. 483 I; \i IB, relation of brain-weight to, ii. 278 Range of accommodation, normal, ii. 312 Rarefied an-, respiration of, i. 152 Kali of conduction in heart muscle, i. 154 in muscles, ii. -7 in nerves, ii. 89 of excitation, effect of. , 4fi, 333 size of. i. :;."> structure of, i. 35 variations in the number of, i. 46 Red-striped muscles, physiological properties of, ii. 10!) Reduced eye, ii. 304 Reduction, i. 502 processes in the animal body. i. 536 Reflex acceleration of the heart, i. 177 actions, simple, ii. 208 arc. ii. 209 coughs, i. 155 discharge of bile, i. 248 frog, ii. 209 inhibition of the heart, i. 172 secretion of gastric juice, i. 239 of pancreatic juice, i. 236 of saliva, i. 230 segmental reaction, ii. 210 stimulation of the nervous system, ii. 208 tonus of muscular tissues, ii. 220 vaso motor changes, i. 202 Reflexes, co-ordinated, ii. 211 co-ordination of the efferent impulses in. ii. 21 1 effect of location of stimulus on, ii. 209 of strength of stimulus on. ii. 210 Relieves from the isolated cord in man, ii. 213 lumbar cord, ii. 213 in different vertebrates, ii. 212 in man, ii. 216 latent period of, ii. 211 of a purposeful character, ii. 215 periodic, ii. 216 simple, ii. 208 spinal, ii. 212 reinforcement of, ii. 222 summation of stimuli in, ii. 211 through sympathetic ganglia, vaso-motor, i. 2()() ' voluntary control of, ii. 214 Refractive index of the aqueous humor, ii. 303 of the lens, ii. 303 of the vitreous humor, ii. 303 media of the eye, ii. 302 surfaces of the eye, ii. 303 " Refractory period" of nerves, ii. 57, 66 of the heart, i. 156, 158 Regeneration of blood after hemorrhage, i. 63 of lost parts, ii. 496 of nerves, ii. 78, 199 Registers of the voice, ii. 432 Regular astigmatism, ii. 317 Reinforcement of reflexes, ii. 222 of the knee-kick, ii. 222 Reissner, membrane of, ii. 374, 379 Rejuvenescence by sexual reproduction, ii. 442 Relaxation of muscle, nature of, ii. 99 Eennin, i. 238 action of, on milk, i. 296 occurrence of, in gastric juice, i. 295 of the kidneys, i. 274 preparation of, i. 295 Reproduction, asexual, ii. 439 of leucophrys patula, ii. 442 of living matter, i. 18, 20 of onychodronius, ii. 442 of stylonichia, ii. 442 sexual, ii. 440 elements of, ii. 440 theory of, ii. 441 Reproductive organs, female, ii. 449 internal secretions of, ii. 462 male, ii. 443 vaso-motor nerves of, i. 208 process, ii. 463 Residual air, definition of, i. 427 Resonance of the ear, ii. 388 Resonants, ii. 436 Resonators, analysis of sounds by, ii. 385 Respiration, artificial, i. 446 associated movements of, i. 408 cutaneous, i. 422 definition of, i. 395 heat dissipated in, i. 488 intensity of. i. 429 internal, i. 422 nervous mechanism of, i. 455 rhythm of. i. 423 Respiratory activity, conditions affecting, i. 429 cent res. i. 155 afferenl nerves to, i. 459 conditions influencing, i. 458 total, i. Mil rhythmicity of. i. 15- food-Stuffs, definition of, i. 346 movements, circulatory effects of, i. 447 duration of, i. I'.'l effect of, on blood-pressure, i. 448 on venous circulation, i. 95, 96 frequency of, i. 125 Bpecial, i. 453 nerves, afferent, i. 460 efferent, i. 463 GENERAL INDEX. 547 Respiratory pauses, i. 424 pressure, i. 408 quotient, i. 410 during hibernation, i. 134 relation of, to the diet, i. 353 variations of, i. 437 sounds, i. 4U!) Resuscitation from drowning, i. 445 Rete mirabile of the Malpighian corpuscles, i. 249 vasculosum of the testis, ii. 447 Retina, changes produced in, by light, ii. 330 circulation in, ii. 322 histology of, ii. 329 oscillatory activity of, ii. 344 space-perceptions by, ii. 348 structure of, ii. 327 Retinal currents, ii. 331 images, inversion of, ii. 305 size of, ii. 305 stimulation, after-effect of, ii. 345 fatigue in, ii. 31 1 latent period of, ii. 343 laws of, ii. 343 rise to maximum for different colors, ii. 313 vessels, demonstration of, ii. 321 Reversion to ancestral characters, ii. 495 Rhamnose, i. 562 Rheocord, ii. 41 Rheometer, i. !)!) Rheonome, ii. 31 Rheoscope, physiological, ii. 148 Rheoseopie frog, ii. 148 Rheostat, ii. 40 Rhineneephalon, ii. 241 Rhomboideus muscles, respiratory action of, i. 405 Rhythm of the respiratory movements, i. 423 Rhythmic activity of the vaso-constrictor cen- tre, i. 201 Rhythmicity of the heart, abnormal, i. 152 cause of, i. 148 Ribs, respiratory movements of. i. 400 Rickets, i. 356, 525 Right lymphatic duct, i. 1 15 Rigor caloris, ii. 57, 161 contracture in, ii. 128 elicit of fatigue on, ii. 165 mortis, ii. 159 chemical changes accompanying, ii. 162 contracture of, ii. 128 disappearance of, ii. 162 influence of the nervous system on, ii. 220 nature of changes in, ii. 161 Rima glottidis. ii. 123 respiratoria, ii. 423 vocalis, ii. 423 Ringer's solution for the heart, i. 190 Hitter's opening tetanus, ii. 37, til tetanus, ii. 132 Rivinus. duds (if, i. -.'17 Rod and-cone layer, function of, ii. 327 Rod-pigment, ii, 339. See Visual purple. Rods and cones, function of, ii. 341 number of, ii. 330 of Corti, ii. 377 ret inal, function of, ii. 311 Rosel von Rosenhofon spontaneous changes in form of living organisms, ii. 19 Rotal ion, movements of, ii. 116 Roy's tonometer, i. L88 Running, ii. 421 Rut of animals, ii. 460 SA.CCH IBOSE, i. 561 Sacculus of the internal ear, ii. 373 Saccus endolymphaticus, ii. 373 Saddle-joint, ii. 116 Saliva, composition of, i. 220, 283 mineral constituents of. i. 53d properties of. i. 220, 283 uses of, i. 286 Salivary corpuscles, i. 283 glands, i. 215 anatomy of, i. 217 histological changes in, i. 226 histology of, i. 219 nerves of, i. 218, '-"-'I vaso-motor nerves of. i. 222 secretion, action of drugs on, i. :.".'!» cerebral control of, ii. 270 normal mechanism of. i. 230 Salkowski's reaction for cholesterin, i. 575 Salmin, i. 580 Salt solution, physiological injection of, i. 64 Salt-licks, i. 355 Salts, absorption of. i. 318 inorganic, of muscle, ii. 168 relation of, to irritability, ii. 58 lymphagogic action of, i. 73 nutritive value of, i. 276, 354 of heavy metals, action of, on nerve and muscle, ii. 60 Santorini, cartilage of, ii. 422, 125 Saponification of fats, i. 306, 558 Saprin, i. 543 Sarcin, i. 553 Sarcode of sponges, contractility of, ii. 20 Sarcolactic acid. i. 546 formation of. in rigor mollis, ii. 161, 162 in clotting of muscle-plasma, ii. 161 relation of, to fatigue contracture, ii. 131 Sarcoplasm, ii. 104 Sarcosin, i. 538 Saturation of colors, ii. 342 Seal a media, ii. 375 tvmpani, ii. 372, 375 vestibuli, ii. 37:.', 375 Scaleni muscles, inspiratory action of, i. 401 Schneiderian membrane, ii. 408 Scombrin, i. 580 Scrotum, ii. 1 13 Sebaceous glands, structure of, i. 257 secretion, composition of. i. 342 function of. i. 258 physiological value of. i. 342 Sebum, composition of. i. "257 Secondary defeneration of nerves, ii. 1!»7 position of the eye, ii. •_':•!• tetanus, ii. 150 Secreting glands, electrical changes in, i. 231 histological changes in, i. \.'\.'6 Secretion, antilytie, i. '.'.".n biliary, i. 248 capillaries of tin- gastric glands, i. 238 definition of. i. 21 1 gastric. •.Mil histological changes during, i. 226 internal, deflnil ion of, i. ".M 1 intest inal. i. 243 mammary, i. 264 mechanism of. i. •.'13 pancreal ic, i. 235 paralytic, i. 229 psychical, of gaBl ric j u ice. i. 239 relation of, to intensity of stimulus, i. 223 salivary, i. 230 cerebral control of, ii. 270 sebaceous, i. '.'57. 342 sweat, i. 259 u rinary. i. 251 Secretions, general characteristics of, i. 213 Secretogogues for the gastric glands, i. 359 Secretory centre, salivary, i. '.'30 548 GENERAL IXDEX. Secretory fibres proper, definition of, i. 224 nerves, evidence for, i. 222 fatigue of, ii. !••> mode of action of, i. 225 of i he adrenal bodies, i. 272 of the kidneys, i. 251 of the liver, i. 247 of the mammary glands, i. 263 of the pancreas, i. 2:52 of tin- stomach, i. 239 of the sweat •.'lands, i. 259 salivary, endings of, i. 220 significance of, i. 214 stimulation of, i. 222 Segmental arrangement of nerve-elements, ii. 206 reactions, reflex, ii. 210 Segmental ion, ii. 167 Segmentation-centrosomes, ii. 469 Segmentation-nucleus, ii. 466 Semen, composition of, ii. I !•"> Semicircular canals, membranous, ii. 373 of t he bony labyrinth, ii. 371 relation of, fco equilibrium, ii. 405 section of, ii. 405 Semilunar valves, i. 1 10 Seminal vesicles, ii. 1 13 fond ion of, ii. 1 18 secretion of, ii. 1 16 Semi-vowels, ii. 136 Senescence of tiel'Ve-cel Is, ii. 182 of tin' central nervous system, ii. 295 phenomena of, ii. 486 Sensation, cutaneous, definition of, ii. 390 muscular, definition of, ii. 390 of after-pressure, ii. 394 of light, mechanism for the production of, ii. 331 of temperature, ii. .",*» Serous cavities, i. 1 16 Senat i postici inferiores, respiratory function of, i. 399 superiores, inspiratory action of, i. 102 Scrum, bactericidal action of, i. 36 globulicidal acl ion of, i. ■'!*> osmol ic pressure of, i. 68 toxicity of, i. 36 Serum-albumin, action of, on carbonates, i. 517 Serum-albumin, amount of, in the blood, i. 52 composit ion of, i 52 functions of, i. 52 properties of, i. 52 Sex, characters of, ii. 442 influence of, on heat production, i. 482 on pulse-rate, i. 121 on respiration, i. 130 of offspring, determination of, ii. 483 origin of, ii. 441 relation of body-temperature to, i. 470 of brain-weigh! to, ii. 276 Sexual characters, ii. 142 glands, accessory, ii. 445 organs, ii. 443 reproduction, ii. 440 congenital variations resulting from, ii. 501 t heory of origin of, ii. 441 Shark, reflexes in, ii. 212 removal of cerebral hemispheres in, ii. 263 Shivering, i. 362, 491 Shrapnell's membrane, ii. 365 Siamese twins, ii. 483 Silicic acid, properties of, i. 519 Silicon, i. 519 Simple muscular contraction, duration of, ii. 102, 108 explanation of. ii. Jul proteids, i. 57o' Simultaneous contrast, ii. 347 Singing voice, ii. 434 Sinuses of Valsalva, i. Ill Size, increase of the embryo in, ii. 4*7 influence of, on pulse-rate, i. 121 of nerve-cells, ii. 175 Skatol, i. 572 elimination of, i. 340 occurrence of, in feces, i. 320 Skiascopy, detection of astigmatism by means of, ii. 319 Skin, functions of, i. 341 glands of, i. 257 tactile areas of, ii. 395 Sleep, ii. 291 cause of, ii. 292 condition of the pupils iu, ii. 325 curve of intensity of, ii. 294 effect of loss of, ii. 295 on metabolism, i. 361 on respiration, i. 424 on the respiratory quotient, i. 438 responsiveness to stimuli in, ii. 293 Smegma prseputii, i. 257 Smell, ii. 408 comparative physiology of, ii. 409 subjective sensations of. ii. 410 Smooth muscle, rate of conduction in, ii. 89 Snails, regeneration of lost parts in, ii. 496 Sneezing, i. 454 Snoring, i. 455 Sobbing, i. 454 Sodium ammonium phosphate, i. 523 carbonates, i. 522, 523 chloride, nutritive history of, i. 521 phosphates, i. 522 sulphate, i. 522 Somatic death, ii. 491 Somatoplasm, definition of, ii. 496 Somatopleure, ii. 472 Sound, phvsieal, ii. 381 quality of, ii. 383 Sound-waves, amplitude of, ii. 381 composite, ii. 384 limits of perceptions of, ii. 382 production of. ii. :\-\ Space illusion-., ii. 35 1 Space-perception from visual sensations, ii. 347 GENERAL INDEX. 549 Special respiratory movements, i. 453 Specialization of functions, i. 21 Specific energies of nerves, doctrine of, ii. 232 energy of the optic nerve, ii. 331 gravity of blood, i. 34 of blood-corpuscles, i. 34, 35 of muscle, ii. 159 of the encephalon, ii. 275 of the nervous system at different ages, ii. 284 of urine, i. 251 heat, definition of, i. 47? of the human body, i. 504 nerve-energy, doctrine of, ii. 399 Spectral colors, incomplete saturation of, ii. 342 Spectroscope, i. 40 Spectrum, ii. 332 definition of, i. 40 luminous intensity of the colors of, ii. 340 of CO-hremoglobin, i. 44 of haemoglobin, i. 42 of oxyhemoglobin, i. 41 solar, i. 41 top, Benham's, ii. 344 Speech, dependence of, on hearing, ii. 431 elements of, ii. 433 Speech-centre, ii. 257 Spermaceti, i. 540 Sperm-aster, ii. 467 Spermatids, ii. 445 Spermatocytes, ii. 445 Spermatozoa, ii. 440 contractility of, ii. 20 discovery of, ii. 443 entrance of, into the uterus, ii. 465 locomotion of, ii. 465 maturation of, ii. 445 movements of, ii. 444 structure of, ii. 443 Spermiu, ii. 445 physiological action of, i. 273 Sperm-nucleus, ii. 466 Spherical aberration, ii. 315 Sphincter ani, cerebral control of, ii. 270 antri pylorici, i. 377 iridis, ii. 325 pylori, i. 377, 381 urethrse, i. 390 contraction of, in erection, ii. Hit vesica? interims, i. 390 Sphincters ani, i. 3*6 Sphygmogram, i. 143 Sphygmograph, i. 1 12 Sphygmomanometer, i. 141 Sphygmometer, i. 141 .Spinal (('ntrcs for vaso-inotor nerves, i. 199 cord, afferent paths of, ii. 229 central neurones of, ii. 203 degeneration of, from heinisect ion, ii. 228 efferent neurones of, ii. 203 motor tracts of, ii. 245 reflexes in man after section of, ii. 213 schematic cross-section of, ii. 202 weight of, ii. 274 ganglion-cells, development of, ii. 178 nerve-roots, section of, ii. 19s Spiral ganglion of the. car, ii. 376 ligament of the cochlea, ii. 379 Spirometer, i. 427 Splanchnic nerves, gastric fibres of, 1.382 influence of, on blood-pressure, i. 173 on respiral ion, i. 463 intestinal fibres of. i. 385 stimulation of, i. 173 Spleen, composition of, i. 333 function of, i. 322 Spleen, innervation of, i. 333 movements of, i. 322 vaso-motor nerves of, i. 207 Staircase contractions, ii. 66, 112 relation of, to tetanus, ii. 124 Standing, ii. 418 Stanniiis's Ligatures, i. L78 Stapedius muscle, ii. 370 Stapes, ii. 367 Starch, i. 566 digestion of, i. 284, 305 hydrolysis of. by acids, i. 286 by amylolytic ferments, i. 2-5 Starvation, effect of. on metabolism, i. 362 on tin- nervous system, ii. 289 glycogen disappearance during, i. 331 nutrition during, i. 350 phosporus excretion in, i. 516 potassium excretion in, i. 520 Stal ic equilibrium, organs of, ii. 407 Stature, relation of brain-weight to, ii. 276 Steapsin, i. 232, 280 demonstration of. i. 306 occurrence of. i. 305 Stearic acid, i. ."11 Stenson's duct, i. 217 experiment, ii. 67 Stercorin, i. 575 Stereoscope, ii. 356 Sterno-cleido-mastoid muscles, respiratorv ac- tion of, i. 404 Sterno-hyoid muscle, ii. 425 Sterno-thyroid muscle, ii. 425 Sternum, respiratory movements of, i. 401 Stethograph, i. 423 Stimulants, effect of, on muscular work, ii. 75 of the sweat glands, i. 260 physiological action of, i. 357 Stimulation fatigue of non-medullated nerves, ii. 180 of the cortex, ii. 211 Stimuli, artificial, effect of, on the heart, i. 156 classification of. ii 23 chemical, of muscle, ii. 131 conditions determining efficiency of, ii. 28 effect of changing intensity of. ii. 32 of their repetition on irritability, ii. 65 of varying strength of. ii. 39 galvanic, contracture effect of, in muscles, ii. 131 variations in intensity of. ii. 31 Stokes's reagent, composition of, i. 43 Stomach, absorption in, i. 312 ex1 irpation of. i. 299 Clauds of, i. 237 immunity of, to its own secretion, i. 297 innervation of, i. ::-l movements of, i. 377, 378 musculature of. i. 377 Strabismus, ii. .".(in Stria' acust icie, ii. 237 gravidarum, ii. 477 Stromuhr of Ludwig, i. 99 St rout ium, i. 526 Strychnin, action of. on diffusion of impulses in Hie cord. ii. 217 on end-plates, ii. '.'7 on sympathetic ganglia, ii. 219 effect of, on body-temperature, i. 172 tetanus, ii. 217 Sturin, i. 580 Stylo-byoid muscle, ii. 126 St ylonychia. reproduction of. ii. 442 Sublingual gland, anatomy of, i. 217 Submaxillary gland, anatomy of. i. 217 Successive contrast, ii. 3 Hi Succinic acid. i. 557 550 GENERAL INDEX. Saccns entericus, i. 243 action of, ou carbohydrates, i. 309 collection of, i. 308 digestive action of, i.308 ferments of, i. 308 Suction action of the heart, i. 134 Sudorific drags, i. 260 Suffocation. See Asphyxia. Sugar injections, lymphagogic action of, i. 73 hi muscles, ii. L67 use of, in muscular work,ii. 74 Sugars, absorption of, i. 313,317 consumption of, by the tissues, i. 353 effect of, on glycogen formation, i. 32ti synthesis of, i. 533 Sulphates of the urine, estimation of, i. 506 origin of, i. .">« >t ; Sulph-haemoglobin, i. 506 Sulphur, elimination of, i. 340 metabolism of, i. 507 neutral, i. 506 occurrence of, i. 505 Sulphuretted hydrogen, inhalation of, i. 440 properties of, i. 506 Sulphuric acid. i. 506 Sulphurous acid. i. 506 Summation of contraction in muscle, ii. 1*21 of stimuli in nerve-cells, ii. 190 in reflex action, ii. 211 Superior laryngeal nerve, ii. 428 nerves, influence of, on respiration, i. 459, 462 oblique muscle, ii. 299 rectus muscle, ii. 299 Supernumerary digits, ii. 494 Supplemental air, definition of. i. 427 Suprarenal capsules, extirpation of, i. 271 Sustentacular cells of the crista acustica, ii. 374 Suture, ii. 414 Swallowing, i. 375 action of the epiglottis in, ii. 422 Sweat, amount of. i. 258, 342 composition of, i. 259, 342 nitrogenous constituents of, i. 512 Sweat-centres, spinal, i. 261 Sweat-glands, secretory nerves of, i. 259 stimulation of, i. 260 structure of, i. 258 Sweat-nerves, i. '.Tilt Sweat -secretion, action of drugs on, i. 260 Sylvian heat-cent re. ii. 271 Sympathetic ganglia, action of nicotin on, ii. 219 of strychnin on, ii. 219 nerves, cardial', i. 168, 171 pulmonary, i. 466 reflex influence of, on the pulse-rate, i. 175 to tlu- iris, ii. 324 pains, ii. 400 secretory fibres to tin- pancreas, i. 232 to die salivary glands, i. 21-. 222 system, connect ion of. with the cerehro-spinal, ii. 21- post-ganglionic fibres of. ii. 219 pre ganglionic fibres of, ii. 218 vaso-motor centres, i. 200 vibration, ii. 3-5 Symphysis, ii. 11 1 Syndesmosis, ii. Ill Synovial fluid, ii 115 Synthesis of proteids, i. 518, 582 of sugars, i. 563 Synthetic processes of plants, i. 518 Syntonin, absorption of. i. 315 occurrence of. in peptic digestion, i 292 Syphilis, hereditary transmission of. ii. 498 Systole, auricular, i. 121. L36 ventricular, i. 123 Tactile anas of the skin, ii, 395 corpuscle, ii. 390, 392 Tartar, i. 524 Taste, nerves of, ii. lln organs of, ii. flu Taste-buds, ii. 410 Taste-nerves, nuclei of origin of, ii. 23d Taste perceptions, conditions affecting, ii. 411 Taste-sensations, conditions which influenc . ii. Ill primary, ii. 412 distribution of. ii. 413 Taurin, i. 507, 543 in muscles, ii. li>7 'Tea, nutritive value of. i. 357 stimulating action of. ii. 75 'Tectorial membrane, ii. 379 Tegmen of the tympanum, ii. 3G4 Temperature, axillary, i. 4d- body-, efl'ect '*'! localization of, ii. 394 Tractus solitarius, ii. 236 Transfusion of blood, i. 64 Transversal is abdominis muscle, respiratory action of. i. 407 Trapezius muscle, respiratory action of, i. 405 Traube-Hering waves, i. 201 Tremors, ii. 132 Triangulares sterni muscles, expiratory action of, i. 107 Trigeminal nerves, central paths of, ii. 238 influence of, on respiration, i. 463 Trimethylamine, i. 511 Trioses, i. 559 Triplets, ii. 483 Trommel's test for carbohydrates, i. 562 Tropseolin 00 test for mineral acid, i. 289 Trophic impulses to muscles, ii. 70 influence of neurones on one another, ii. 197 of the vagi on the heart, i. 167 nerves of the muscles, ii. 70 of the salivary glands, i. 224 pulmonary, i. 4iii! Trypsin, i. 232 effect of, on blood coagulation, i. 63 extracts, preparation of, i. 301 properties of, i. 301 Tripsinogen, i. 235 granules, i. 235 Tryptie digestion, products of, i. 302 value of. i. 304 Tryptophan, i. 574 Tubules, uriniferous, i. 250 Tubuli recti of the testis, ii. 447 Tunicin, i. 566 Turtle's striped muscle, time of contraction in. ii. 108 Twins, ii. 1*2 Tympanic membrane, ii. 364 effect of destruction of, ii. 370 pressure-sensations of, ii. 382 vibrations of. ii. 370 Tympanum, ii. 363 mechanics of, ii. 368 Tyrosin, i. 570 formation of, in tryptie digestion, i. 303 Ultimum moriens, ii. 492 Umbilical arteries, ii. 474 vein, ii. 17 I Umbo of the tympanic membrane, ii. 365 Unconsciousness, ii. 293 Unipolar excitation for localized excitation, ii. 15 nerve-cells, development of, ii. 17- stimulation, ii. 30 principles of, ii. 13 Unite, calorimel ric, i. 177 Unorganized ferments, definition of. i. 271' Urea, amount of, in sweat, i. .'!:;."> in the bl 1, i. 51 in the mine, i. :;:',."> antecedents of', i. ::::."> eliminal ion of. i. 252 est imaf ion of, i. ."> lit formation of. after removal of the liver, i. 3:57 in f he liver, i. :;::i in muscles, ii. L67 origin of, in the body. i. 550 in the liver, i. 266 preparation of, i. ."> i^ from proteid, i. 337 presence of, in sweat, i. .'!.".7 proper! ies of, i. .". |<> Ureters, movements of, j. 371, 389 552 G ENEMA L IX J) EX. Urethra, ii. 443 structure of, ii. 448 Qric acid, formation of, i. 338 in the liver, i. 322 in the spleen, i. 333 in muscles, ii. lt>7 molecular structure of, i. 554 occurrence "t', i. 338 origin of, in birds, i. 557 in mammals, i. 338, 556 preparal ion of, i. 555 proper! ies of, i. 555 Urinary bladder, innervation of, i. 392 movements of, i. 390 pigments, origin of, from haemoglobin, i. 45 se< retion. normal stimulus for, i. 255 relation of, to the blood-flow through the kidney, i. 253 Urine, acidity of, after meals, i. 290 composition of, i. 250, 334 et In real sulphates of, i. 572 secretion of, i. 251 Uriniferous tubules, secretory function of, i. 252 -t met ure of, i. 250 Urobilin, i. 57 1 1 'terns, ii. 1 13, 15<> Utri cuius of the internal ear, ii. 373 Vagina, ii. 443, 162 Vagus, anabolic action of, on the heart, i. 166 anatomy of, in the dog, i. 159 cardiac branches of, i. 159 central path of the afferent fibres in, ii. 236 etl'eet on the heart, nature of, i. 166 gastric branches of, i. 381 inhibition, dependence of, on the character of the stimulus, i. L65 intestinal branches of, i. 385 nerve, fatigue of. ii. 96 pulmonary branches of, i. 465 rate of conduction in, ii. 90 relation of, to apncea, i 442 respiratory function of, i. 459 pneumonia, i. 166 secretory fibres ,.f. to the pancreas, i. 232 to the stomach, i. 239 stimulation, auricular effects of, i. 164 effect of, latent period of, i. 162 on the heart, i. L52, 163 on the ventricle, i. L62 Valsalva's experiment, i. 152 sinuses, i. Ill Valves, auriculo ventricular, i. 108 of lymphatic vessels, i. 146 .semilunar, i. 110 Valvuhe conniventes, value of. in absorption, i. :;n Variation of the offspring in reproduction, ii. 500 Variations, somatic, classification of, i. 197 Vas deferens, ii. 1 17 Yasa deferentia, ii. I !•"> efferentia of the testis, ii. 1 17 Vaseline, i. 531 Vaso-constrictor centre, rhythmical activity of, i. -.'(il. 151 nerves, discovery of. i. 19.'! Vaso-dilatoT nerves, discovery of. i. mi Vaso-motor centre, medullary, i. 198 centres, spinal, i. lii!» sympal be tic, i. '.'on nerves, anatomy of. i. 198 methods of investigating, i. 195 of the brain, i. 203 of the cranial vessels, ii. 2*6 of the generative organs, i. 208 Vaso-motor nerves of the head, i. 204 of the heart, i. 206 of the intestines, i. 206 of tin- kidneys, i. 207, 256 of the limbs, i. 209 of the liver, i. 206 of the luugs, i. 205, 466 of the muscles, i. 2 in of the pancreas, i. 207 of the portal system, i. 209 of the salivary glands, i. 222 of tin- spleen, i. 207 of the tongue, i. 205 of t he veins, i. 195 special properties of, i. 197 reflexes, i. 201 through the vagi, i. 172 Vegetable food.-, composition of, i. 278 proteids, i. 577 Veins, ethct of compression of, on lymph forma- tion, i . 72 entrance of air into. i. 97 rate of flow in. i. in] vaso-motor nerves of, i. 209 Velocity of blood-flow, i. 99-101 Vense Thebesii, i. 184 Veno-motor nerves of the limbs, i. 209 Venous blood-flow, effect of the auricles on, i. L37 circulation, i. 95, 96 pressure, i. 91, 94 pulse, respiratory, i. 96 Ventilation, principles of, i. 439 Ventral nerve-roots, number of fibres of, ii. 230 Ventricles, independent rhythm of, i. 152 of Morgagni, ii. 422 of the hrain. capacity of, ii. 271 work done by, i. 106, 107 Ventricular hands, ii. 422 cycle, analysis of, i. 133 diastole, duration of. i. 123 pressure-curves, analysis of, i. 128 pressures, i. 125 systole, duration of, i. 123 Veratria, action of, on coagulation of muscle- plasma, ii. 164 on muscular contraction, ii. 129. 137 on nerves and muscles, ii. 60 effect of. on muscular contraction, ii. 128 Vernix caseosa, i. 258 Vessels of Thebesius, i. 186 Vertigo in diseases of the ear labyrinth, ii. 406 Vestibular root of the auditory nerve, central path of. ii. 237 Vestibule of the bony labyrinth, ii. 371 Vibrations of the tympanic membrane, ii. 370 transmission of, through the labyrinth, ii. :;7 CO. Chapin on Insanity. A Compendium of Insanity. By John B. Chapin, M. D., LL. D., Physician-in-Chief, Pennsylvania Hospital for the Insane ; Honorary Member of the Medico-Psychological Society of Great Britain, of the Society of Mental Medicine of Belgium, etc. 121110, 234 pages, illus- trated. Cloth, $1.25 net. Chapman's Medical Jurisprudence arid Toxicology. Second Edition, Revised. Medical Jurisprudence and Toxicology. By Henry C. Chapman, M. D., Professor of Institutes of Medicine and Medical Jurisprudence, Jefferson Medical College of Philadelphia. 254 pages, with 55 illus- trations and 3 full-page plates in colors. Cloth, $1 .50 net. Church arid Peterson's Nervous arid Mental Diseases. Third Edition, Revised and Enlarged. Nervous and Mental Diseases. By Archibald Church, M. D., Pro- fessor of Nervous and Mental Diseases, and Head of the Neurological Department, Northwestern University Medical School, Chicago; and Frederick Peterson, M. D., Chief of Clinic, Nervous Department, College of Physicians and Surgeons, New York. Handsome octavo volume of 875 pages, profusely illustrated. Cloth, $5.00 net; Sheep or Half Morocco, $6.00 net. Clarkson's Histology. A Text-Book of Histology, Descriptive and Practical. By Arthur Clarkson, M. B., C. M. Edin., formerly Demonstrator of Physiology in the Owen's College, Manchester ; late Demonstrator of Physiology in Yorkshire College, Leeds. Large octavo, 554 pages; 22 engravings and 174 beautifully colored original illustrations. Cloth, #4.00 net. Corwin's Physical Diagnosis. Third Edition, Revised. Essentials of Physical Diagnosis of the Thorax. By Arthur M. Corwin, A.M., M. D., Instructor in Physical Diagnosis in Rush Medical College, Chicago. 219 pages, illustrated. Cloth, Si. 25 net. DaCosta's Surgery. Third Edition. Revised. Modern Surgery, General and Operative. By John Chalmers Da Costa, M. D., Professor of Principles of Surgery and Clinical Surgery, Jefferson Medical College, Philadelphia : Surgeon to the Philadelphia Hospital, etc. Handsome octavo volume of 11 17 pages, profusely illustrated. Cloth, $5.00 net; Sheep or Half Morocco, $6.00 net. Enlarged by over 200 Pages, with more than 100 New Illustrations. Davis's Obstetric Nursing. Obstetric and Gynecologic Nursing. By Edward P. Davis, A.M., M. 1)., Professor of Obstetrics, Jefferson Medical College and Phila- delphia Polyclinic; Obstetrician and Gynecologist, Philadelphia Hos- pital. 121110, 400 pages, illustrated. Crushed Buckram, si. 75 net. MEDICAL PUBLICATIONS DeSchweinitz on Diseases of the Eye. Third Edition, Revised. Diseases of the Eye. A Handbook of Ophthalmic Practice. By (1. E. de S< iiHiiMi1/, M. I)., Professor of < >phthalmology, Jefferson Medi- cal College, Philadelphia, etc. Handsome royal octavo volume of 696 pages; 256 fine illustrations and 2 chromodithographic plates. Cloth, 54.00 net; Sheep or Half Morocco. 55.00 net. Dorland's Dictionaries. [See American Illustrated Medical Dictionary and American Pocket Medical Dictionary on page 3.] Dorland'S Obstetrics. Second Edition, Revised and Greatly Enlarged. Modern Obstetrics. By W. A. Newman Dorland, M. D., Assistant Demonstrator of Obstetrics, University of Pennsylvania ; Associate in Gynecology, Philadelphia Polyclinic. Octavo volume of 797 pages, with 201 illustrations. Cloth, $4.00 net. Eichhorst's Practice of Medicine. A Text- Book of the Practice of Medicine. By Dr. Herman Eichhorst, Professor of Special Pathology and Therapeutics and Director of the Medical Clinic, University of Zurich. Translated and edited by Augus- tus A. Eshnkk, M. D., Professor of Clinical Medicine, Philadelphia Polyclinic. Two octavo volumes of 600 pages each, over 150 illustrations. Prices per set: Cloth, $6.00 net; Sheep or Half Morocco, S7.50 net. Friedrich arid Curtis on the Nose, Throat, and Ear. Rhinology, Laryngology, and Otology, and Their Significance in Gen- eral Medicine. By Dr. E. P. Friedrich, of Leipzig. Edited by H. Holbrook Curtis, M. D., Consulting Surgeon to the New York Nose and Throat Hospital. Octavo, 348 pages. Cloth, $2.50 net. Frothingham's Guide for the Bacteriologist. Laboratory Guide for the Bacteriologist. By Langdon Frothingham, M. D. V., Assistant in Bacteriology and Veterinary Science, Sheffield Scientific School, Vale University. Illustrated. Cloth, 75 cts. net. Garrigues' Diseases of Women. Third Edition, Revised. Diseases of Women. By Henry J. Garrigues, A. M., M. D., Gyne- cologist to St. Mark's Hospital and to the German Dispensary, New York City. Octavo, 756 pages, with 367 engravings and colored plates. Cloth, $4.50 net; Sheep or Half Morocco, S550 net. Gorham's Bacteriology. A Laboratory Course in Bacteriology. By I'. I'. Gorham, M. A., Assistant Professor in biology. Brown University. t2mo volume of 192 pages, 97 illustrations. Cloth, #1.25 net. OF W. B. SAUNDERS & CO. Gould and Pyle's Curiosities of Medicine. Anomalies and Curiosities of Medicine. By George M. Gould, M.D., and Walter L. Pyle, M. I). An encyclopedic collection of rare and extraordinary cases and of the most striking instances of abnormality in all branches of Medicine and Surgery, derived from an exhaustive research of medical literature from its origin to the present claw abstracted, classified, annotated, and indexed. Handsome octavo volume of 968 pages; 295 engravings and 12 full-page plates. Popular Edition. Cloth, £3.00 net; Sheep or Half Morocco, $4.00 net. Grafstrom's Mechano-Therapy. A Text-Book of Mechano-Therapy (Massage and Medical Gymnastics). By Axel V. Grafstrom, B. Sc, M. D., late House Physician, City Hos- pital, Blackwell's Island, New York. 121110, 139 pages, illustrated. Cloth, $1.00 net. Griffith OI1 the Baby. Second Edition, Revised. The Care of the Baby. By J. P. Crozer Griffith, M. D., Clinical Professor of Diseases of Children, University of Pennsylvania ; Phy- sician to the Children's Hospital, Philadelphia, etc. i2mo, 404 pages; 67 illustrations and 5 plates. Cloth, $1.50 net. Griffith's Weight Chart. Infant's Weight Chart. Designed by J. P. Crozer Griffith, M. D., Clinical Professor of Diseases of Children, University of Pennsylvania. 25 charts in each pad. Per pad, 50 cts. net. Hart's Diet in Sickness and in Health. Diet in Sickness and Health. By Mrs. Ernest Hart, formerly Student of the Faculty of Medicine of Paris and of the London School of Medi- cine for Women ; with an Introduction by Sir Henrv Thompson, F. R. C. S., M. D., London. 220 pages. Cloth, $1.50 net. Haynes' Anatomy. A Manual of Anatomy. By Irving S. Havnes, M. D., Professor of Practical Anatomy in Cornell University Medical College. 680 pages ; 42 diagrams and 134 full-page half-tone illustrations from original photo- graphs of the author's dissections. Cloth, 32.50 net. Heisler's Embryology, second Edition, Revised. A Text-Book of Embryology. By John C. Heisler, M. IV, Professor of Anatomy, Medico-Chirurgical College. Philadelphia. Octavo volume of 405 pages, handsomely illustrated. Cloth, 32.50 net. Hirst'S Obstetrics. Third Edition, Revised and Enlarged. A Text- Hook of < Obstetrics. By Barton < Iooke I [irst, M. D., Professor of Obstetrics, University of Pennsylvania. Handsome octavo volume of 873 pages ; 704 illustrations, 36 of them in colors. Cloth, S5.00 net ; Sheep or Half Morocco, 36.00 net. MEDICAL PUBLIC A TIONS Hyde arid Montgomery on Syphilis arid the Venereal Diseases. Second Edition, Revised and Greatly Enlarged. Syphilis and the Venereal Diseases. By James Nevins Hyde, M. D., Professor of Skin and Venereal Diseases, and Frank H. Montgomery, M. 1)., Associate Professor of Skin, Genito-Urinary, and Venereal Dis- eases in Rush Medical College, Chicago, 111. Octavo, 594 pages, profusely illustrated. Cloth, $4.00 net. 'The International Text- Book of Surgery. i„ Two volumes. By American and British Authors. Edited by 1. Collins Warren, ML D., LL. D., F. R. C. S. (Hon.), Professor of Surgery, Harvard Medi- cal School, Boston ; and A. Pearce Gould, M. S., F. R. C. S., Lecturer on Practical Surgery and Teacher of Operative Surgery, Middlesex Hospital Medical School, London, Eng. Vol. I. General Surgery. — Handsome octavo, 947 pages, with 458 beautiful illustrations and 9 lithographic plates. Vol. II. Special or Regional Surgery. — Handsome octavo, 1072 pages, with 471 beautiful illustrations and 8 lithographic plates. Sold by Subscription. Prices per volume: Cloth, $5.00 net; Sheep or Half Morocco, $6. 00 net. " It is the most valuable work on the subject that has' appeared in some years. The clini- cian and the pathologist have joined hands in its production, and the result must be a satis- faction to the editors as it is a gratification to the conscientious reader." — Annals of Surgery. " This is a work which comes to us on its own intrinsic merits. Of the latter it has very many. The arrangement of subjects is excellent, and their treatment by the different authors is equally so. What is especially to be recommended is the painstaking endeavor of each writer to make his subject clear and to the point. To this end particularly is the technique of operations lucidly described in all necessary detail. And withal the work is up to date in a very remarkable degree, many of the latest operations in the different regional parts of the body being given in full details. There is not a chapter in the work from which the reader may not learn something new." — Medical Record, New York. Jackson's Diseases of the Eye. A Manual of Diseases of the Eye. By Edward Jackson, A. M., M. D., Emeritus Professor of Diseases of the Eye, Philadelphia Polyclinic and College for Graduates in Medicine. 121110 volume of 535 pages, with 178 illustrations, mostly from drawings by the author. Cloth, $2.50 net. Keating's Life Insurance. How to Examine for Life Insurance. By John M. Keating, M. D., Fellow of the College of Physicians of Philadelphia ; Ex-President of the Association of Life Insurance Medical Directors. Royal octavo, 211 pages. "With numerous illustrations. Cloth, S2.00 net. Keen on the Surgery of Typhoid Fever. The Surgical Complications and Sequels of Tvphoid Fever. By Wm. W. Kjeen, M. I)., LI, D., F. R. C. S. (Hon.), Professor of the Principles of Surgery and of Clinical Surgery, Jefferson Medical College, Phila- delphia, etc. Octavo volume of 386 pages, illustrated. Cloth, $3.00 net. Keen's Operation Blank. Second Edition. Revised Form. An Operation Blank, with Lists of Instruments, etc., Required in Arari- ous Operations. Prepared by W. W. Keen, M. D., LL. D., F. R. C. S. (Hon.), Professor of the Principles of Surgery and of Clinical Surgery, Jefferson Medical College, Philadelphia. Price per pad, blanks for fifty operations, 50 cts. net. OF W. B. SAUNDERS &> CO. Kyle on the Nose and Throat, second Edition. Diseases of the Nose and Throat. By D. Braden Kyle, M. D., Clinical Professor of Laryngology and Rhinology, Jefferson Medical College, Philadelphia. Octavo, 646 pages; over 150 illustrations and 6 litho- graphic plates. Cloth, $4.00 net; Sheep or Half Morocco, $5.00 net. Laine's Temperature Chart. By D. T. Laine, M. D. For recording Temperature, with columns for daily amounts of Urinary and Fecal Excretions, Food, etc. ; with the Brand Treatment of Typhoid Fever on the back of each chart. Pad of 25 charts, 50 cts. net. Levy, Klemperer, and Eshner's Clinical Bacteriology. The Elements of Clinical Bacteriology. By Dr. Ernst Levy, Pro- fessor in the University of Strasburg, and Felix Klemperer, Privat- docent in the University of Strasburg. Translated and edited by Augustus A. Eshner, M. D., Professor of Clinical Medicine, Philadel- phia Polyclinic. Octavo, 440 pages, fully illustrated. Cloth, $2.50 net. Lockwood's Practice cf Medicine. tJSHSSU A Manual of the Practice of Medicine. By George R oe Lockwood, M. D., Attending Physician to Bellevue Hospital, New York. Octavo, 847 pages, illustrated, including 22 colored plates. Cloth, 54.00 net. Long's Syllabus of Gynecology. A Syllabus of Gynecology, arranged in Conformity with "An American Text-Book of Gynecology." By J. W. Long, M. D., Professor of Dis- eases of Women and Children, Medical College of Virginia, etc. Cloth, interleaved, $1.00 net. Macdonald's Surgical Diagnosis and Treatment. Surgical Diagnosis and Treatment. By J. W. Macdonald, M. D. Edin., F. R. C. S. Edin., Professor of Practice of Surgery and Clinical Surgery, Hamline University. Handsome octavo, 800 pages, fully illus- trated. Cloth, 55.00 net; Sheep or Half Morocco, $6.00 net. Mallory and Wright's Pathological Technique. Second Edition, Revised. Pathological Technique. A Practical Manual for Laboratory Work in Pathology, Bacteriology, and Morbid Anatomy, with chapters on Post- Mortem Technique and the Performance of Autopsies. By Frank B. Mallory, A. M., M. D., Assistant Professor of Pathology, Harvard University Medical School, Boston; and James H. Wright, A.M., M. D., Instructor in Pathology, Harvard University Medical School, Boston. Octavo, 432 pages, fully illustrated. Cloth, $3.00 net. McClellan's Anatomy in its Relation to Art. Anatomy in its Relation to Art. An Exposition of the bones and Muscles' of the Human Body, with Reference to their Influence upon its Actions and External Form. Bj George McClellan, M.D., Professor of Anatomy, Pennsylvania Academy of Fine Arts Hand- some quarto volume, 9 b\ ti^ inches. Illustrated with 338 original drawings and photographs; 260 pages of text. Dark Blue Vellum, $10.00 net; Half Russia, $12. OO net. MEDICAL PUB LIC A TIOSS McClellan's Regional Anatomy. Regional Anatomy in its Relations to Medicine and Surgery. By George M< Cli i i an. M. I)., Professor of Anatomy. Pennsylvania Acad- emy of Fine Arts. Two handsome quarto volumes, 884 pages of text, and 97 fuU-page chromo-lithographic plates, reproducing the author's original disse< nous. Cloth, S12.00 net ; Half Russia, 815.00 net. McFarland's Pathogenic Bacteria. ™£g%2 %?%£!' Text-Book upon the Pathogenic Bacteria. By Joseph M< Farland, M.D.. Professor of Pathology and Bacteriology, Medico-Chirurgical College of Philadelphia, etc. Octavo volume of 621 pages, finely illustrated. Cloth, S3. 25 net. Meigs on Feeding in Infancy. Feeding in Early Infancy. By Arthur V. Meigs, M. D. Bound in limp cloth, flush edges, 25 cts. net. Moore's Orthopedic Surgery. A Mantial of Orthopedic Surgery. By James E. Moore, M. D., Pro- fessor of Orthopedics and Adjunct Professor of Clinical Surgery, Uni- versity of Minnesota, College of Medicine and Surgery. Octavo volume of 356 pages, handsomely illustrated. Cloth, $2.50 net. Morten's Nurses' Dictionary. Nurses' Dictionary of Medical Terms and Nursing Treatment. Con- taining Definitions of the Principal Medical and Nursing Terms and Abbreviations ; of the Instruments, Drugs, Diseases, Accidents, Treat- ments, Operations, Foods, Appliances, etc. encountered in the ward or in the sick-room. By Honnor Morten, author of "How to Become a Nurse," etc. i6mo, 140 pages. Cloth, #1.00 net. Nancrede's Anatomy and Dissection. Fourth Edition. Essentials of Anatomy and Manual of Practical Dissection. By Charles B. Nancrede, M. D., LL. D., Professor of Surgery and of Clinical Sur- gery, University of Michigan. Ann Arbor. Post-octavo, 500 pages, with full-page lithographic plates in colors and nearly 200 illustrations. Extra Cloth (or Oilcloth for dissection-room), 52.00 net. Nancrede's Principles of Surgery. Lectures on the Principles of Surgery. By Chas. B. Nancrede, M. D., LL. D., Professor of Surgery and of Clinical Surgery, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor. Octavo, 398 pages, illustrated. Cloth, $2.50 net. Norris's Syllabus of Obstetrics. Third Edition. Revised. Syllabus of Obstetrical Lectures in the Medical Department of the University of Pennsylvania. By Richard C. Norris, A. M., M. D., Instructor in Obstetrics and Lecturer on Clinical and Operative Obstet- rics, University of Pennsylvania. Crown octavo, 222 pages. Cloth, interleaved for notes, $2.00 net. OF W. B. SAUNDERS cV CO. Ogden on the Urine. Clinical Examination of the Urine and Urinary Diagnosis. A Clinical Guide for the Use of Practitioners and Students of Medicine and Sur- gery. By J. Bergen Ogden, M. I)., Instructor in Chemistry. Harvard Medical School. Handsome octavo, 416 pages, with 54 illustrations and a number of colored plates. Cloth, $3.00 net. Penrose's Diseases of Women. Fourth Edition, Revised. A Text-Book of Diseases of Women. By Charles B. Penrose, M. D., Ph. D., formerly Professor of Gynecology in the University of Penn- sylvania. Octavo volume of 538 pages, handsomely illustrated. Cloth, $3-75 net- Pryor — Pelvic Inflammations. The Treatment of Pelvic Inflammations through the Vagina. By W. R. Pryor, M. D., Professor of Gynecology, New York Polyclinic. i2mo, 248 pages, handsomely illustrated. Cloth, §2.00 net. Pye's Bandaging. Elementary Bandaging and Surgical Dressing. With Directions con- cerning the Immediate Treatment of Cases of Emergency. By Walter Pye, F. R. C. S., late Surgeon to St. Mary's Hospital, London. Small i2mo, over 80 illustrations. Cloth, flexible covers, 75 cts. net. Pyle's Personal Hygiene. A Manual of Personal Hygiene. Proper Living upon a Physiologic Basis. Edited by Walter L. Pyle, M. D., Assistant Surgeon to the Wills Eye Hospital, Philadelphia. Octavo volume of 344 pages, fully illustrated. Cloth, 51.50 net. Raymond's Physiology. R^f^ifr&d. A Text-Book of Physiology. By Joseph H. Raymond, A. M., M. D., Professor of Physiology and Hygiene in the Long Island College Hospital, and Director of Physiology in Hoagland Laboratory, New- York. Octavo, 668 pages, 443 illustrations. Cloth, #3.50 net. Salinger arid Kalteyer's Modern Medicine. Modern Medicine. By Julius L. Salinger, M. D., Demonstrator of Clinical Medicine, Jefferson Medical College; and F. J. Kai iivir, M. D., Assistant Demonstrator of Clinical Medicine, Jefferson Medical College. Handsome octavo, 801 pages, illustrated. Cloth, 54.00 net. Saundby's Renal arid Urinary Diseases. Lectures on Renal and Urinary Diseases. By Robert Saundbv, M. D. Edin., Fellow of the Royal College of Physicians, London, and of the Royal Medico-Chirurgical Society; Professor of Medicine in Mason College, Birmingham, etc. Octavo, 434 pages, with numerous illustra- tions and 4 colored plates. Cloth, $2.50 net. Saunders' Medical Hand-Atlases. See pages 16 and 17. i2 MEDICAL PUBLICATIONS Saunders* Pocket Medical Formulary, sixth Edition. Revised. Bv William M. Powell, M. D., author of "Essentials of Diseases of Children": Member of Philadelphia Pathological Society. Contain- ing 1844 formulae from the best-known authorities. With an Appendix containing Posoiogical Table, Formulae and Doses for Hypodermic Medication, Poisons and their Antidotes, Diameters of the Female Pelvis and Fetal Head, Obstetrical Table, Diet Lists, Materials and Drugs used in Antiseptic Surgery. Treatment of Asphyxia from Drowning, Sur- gical Remembrancer, Tablesof Incompatibles, Eruptive Fevers, etc., etc. In flexible morocco, with side index, wallet, and flap. $2.00 net. Saunders* Question-Compends. see page 15= Scudder's Fractures, second Edition, Revised. The Treatment of Fractures. By Chas. L. Scudder, M. D., Assistant in Clinical and Operative Surgery, Harvard University Medical School. Octavo, 460 pages, with nearly 600 original illustrations. Polished Buckram, $4. 50 net; Half Morocco, $5.50 net. Senn's Genito-Urinary Tuberculosis. Tuberculosis of the Genito-Urinary Organs, Male and Female. By Nicholas Senn, M. D., Ph. D., LL. D., Professor of the Practice of Surgery and of Clinical Surgery, Rush Medical College, Chicago. Handsome octavo volume of 320 pages, illustrated. Cloth, $3.00 net. Senn's Practical Surgery. Practical Surgery. By Nicholas Senn, M. D., Ph. D., LL. D., Pro- fessor of the Practice of Surgery and of Clinical Surgery, Rush Medical College, Chicago. Octavo, 1133 pages, 642 illustrations. Cloth, $6.00 net : Sheep or Half Morocco, S7.C0 net. By Subscription. Senn's Syllabus qf Surgery. A Syllabus of Lectures on the Practice of Surgery, arranged in con- formity with "An American Text-Book of Surgery." By Nicholas Senn, M. D., Ph. D., LL. D., Professor of the Practice of Surgery and of Clinical Surgery, Rush Medical College, Chicago. Cloth, $1.50 net. Senn's Tumors. Second Edition. Revised. Pathology and Surgical Treatment of Tumors. By Nicholas Senn, M. D. , Ph. D., LL. D. , Professor of the Practice of Surgery and of Clinical Surgery, Rush Medical College, Chicago. Octavo volume of 718 pages, with 47S illustrations, including 12 full-page plates in colors. Cloth, S5.00 net ; Sheep or Half Morocco, $6.00 net. Sollmann's Pharmacology. A Text-Hook of Pharmacology : including Therapeutics, Materia Medica, Pharmacy, Prescription-Writing, Toxicology, etc 15}- Torald Soll- mann, M. I)., Assistant Professor of Pharmacology and Materia Medica, Western Reserve University, Cleveland, Ohio. Handsome octavo, 894 pages, fully illustrated. Cloth, S3. 75 net. OF W. B. SAUNDERS & CO. 13 Starr's Diets for Infants arid Children. Diets for Infants and Children in Health and in Disease. By Louis Starr, M. D., Editor of "An American Text-Book of the Diseases of Children." 230 blanks (pocket-book size), perforated and neatly bound in flexible morocco. $1.25 net. Stengel's Pathology. Third Edition, Thoroughly Revised. A Te,xt-Book of Pathology. By Alfred Stengel, M. D., Professor of Clinical Medicine, University of Pennsylvania ; Visiting Physician to the Pennsylvania Hospital. Handsome octavo, 873 pages, nearly 400 illustrations, many of them in colors. Cloth, $5.00 net; Sheep or Half Morocco, §6.00 net. Stengel arid White on the Blood. The Blood in its Clinical and Pathological Relations. By Alfred Stengel, M. D., Professor of Clinical Medicine, University of Penn- sylvania ; and C. Y. White, Jr., M. D., Instructor in Clinical Medicine, University of Pennsylvania. In Press. Stevens' Therapeutics. ^'StJ^fJytZ^. A Text-Book of Modern Therapeutics. By A. A. Stevens, A. M., M. D., Lecturer on Physical Diagnosis in the University of Pennsylvania. Stevens' Practice of Medicine. Fifth Edition, Revised. A Manual of the Practice of Medicine. By A. A. Stevens, A. M., M. D., Lecturer on Physical Diagnosis in the University of Pennsyl- vania. Specially intended for students preparing for graduation and hospital examinations. Post-octavo, 519 pages; illustrated. Flexible Leather, $2.00 net. Stewart's Physiology. Fourth Edition, Revised. A Manual of Physiology, with Practical Exercises. For Students and Practitioners. By G. N. Stewart, M. A., M. D., D. Sc, Professor of Physiology in the Western Reserve University, Cleveland, Ohio. Octavo volume of 894 pages; 336 illustrations and 5 colored plates. Cloth, #3-75 net. Stoney's Materia Medica for Nurses. Materia Medica for Nurses. By Emily A. M. S rONEY, late Superintend- ent of the Training-School for Nurses. Carney Hospital, South Boston, Mass. Handsome octavo volume of 306 pages. Cloth, $1.50 net. Stoney's Nursing. Second Edition, Revised. Practical Points in Nursing. For Nurses in Private Practice. By Emily A. M. Stoney, late Superintendent of the Training-School for Nurses, Carney Hospital, South Boston, Mass. 456 pages, with 73 engravings and 8 colored and half-tone plates. Cloth, $1.75 net. Stoney's Surgical Technic for Nurses. Bacteriology and Surgical Technic for Nurses. By EMU V A. M. STONEY, late Superintendent of the Training-School for Nurses, Carney Hospital, South Boston, Mass. 121110 volume, fully illustrated. Cloth, 51.25 net. 14 MEDICAL PUBLICATIONS. Thomas's Diet ListS. Second Edition, Revised. Diet Lists and Sick-Room Dietary. By Jerome B. Thomas, M. D., Visiting Physician to the Home for Friendlecs Women and Children and to the Newsboys' Home ; Assistant Visiting Physician to the Kings County Hospital. Cloth. £1.25 net. Send f r sample sheet. Thornton's Dose- Book and Prescription-Writing. Second Edition, Revised and Enlarged. Dose-Book and Manual of Prescription- Writing. By E. Q. Thornton, M. H.. Demonstrator of Therapeutics, Jefferson Medical College, Phila. Post-octavo, 362 pages, illustrated. Flexible Leather, £2.00 net. Vecki'S SeXUai Impotence. Third Edition, Revised and Enlarged. The Pathology and Treatment of Sexual Impotence. By Victor G. Vixki, M. 1). From the second German edition, revised and enlarged. Demi-octavo, 329 pages. Cloth, $2.00 net. Vierordt's Medical Diagnosis. Fourth Edition, Revised. Medical I )iagnosis. By Dr. Oswald Vierordt, Professor of Medicine, University of Heidelberg. Translated, with additions, from the fifth enlarged German edition, with the author's permission, by Francis H. Stuart, A.M., M. D. Handsome octavo volume, 603 pages; 194 wood-cuts, many of them in colors. Cloth, $4.00 net; Sheep or Half Morocco, S5.00 net. Watson's Handbook for Nurses. A Handbook for Nurses. By J. K. Watson, M. D. Edin. American Edition, under supervision of A. A. Stevens, A. M., M. D., Lecturer on Physical Diagnosis, University of Pennsylvania. 121110, 413 pages, 73 illustrations. Cloth, $1.50 net. Warren's Surgical Pathology, second Edition. Surgical Pathology and Therapeutics. By John Collins Warren, M. D., LL. D., F. R. C. S. (Hon.), Professor of Surgery, Harvard Medical School. Handsome octavo, 873 pages; 136 relief and litho- graphic illustrations, ^ in colors. With an Appendix on Scientific Aids to Surgical Diagnosis, and a series of articles on Regional Bacte- riology. Cloth, $5.00 net; Sheep or Half Morocco, $6.00 net. Warwick arid Tunstall's First Aid to the Injured arid Sick. First Aid to the Injured and Sick. By F. J. Warwick, B. A., M. B., Cantab., M.R. ('. S., Surgeon-Captain, Volunteer Medical Staff Corps, London Companies; and A. C Tunstall, M.D.. F. R. C. S. Ed., Surgeon-Captain commanding Fast London Volunteer Brigade Bearer Company. 161110, 232 pages; nearly 200 illustrations, (loth, vsi.oo net. Wolfs Examination of Urine. A Handbook of Physiologic Chemistry and Urine Examination. By Chas. G. F. Wolf, M. I>., Instructor in Physiologic Chemistry. Cornell University Medical College. 12110, 204 pages, illustrated. Cloth, $1.25 net. SAUNDERS* QUESTION-COMPEND SERIES. Price, Cloth, $1.00 net per copy, except when otherwise noted. *' Where the work of preparing students' manuals is to end we cannot say, but the Saunders Series, in our opinion, bears off the palm at present." — New York Medical Record. 1. Essentials of Physiology. By Sidney BUDGETT, M. D. A New Work. 2. Essentials of Surgery. By Edward MARTIN, M.D. Seventh edition, revised, with an Appendix and a chapter on Appendicitis. 3. Essentials of Anatomy. By Chaki.es B. Nancrede, M. D. Sixth edition, thor- oughly revised and enlarged. 4. Essentials of Medical Chemistry, Organic and Inorganic. By Lawrence Wolff, M. D. Fifth edition, revised. 5. Essentials of Obstetrics. By W. Easterly Ashton, M. I). Fourth edition, rerised and enlarged. 6. Essentials of Pathology and Morbid Anatomy. By F. 1. Kalteyer, M.D. In preparation. 7. Essentials of Materia Medica, Therapeutics, and Prescription-Writing. By Henry Morris, M. D. Fifth edition, revised. 8. 9. Essentials of Practice of Medicine. By Henry Morris, M. D. An Appendix on Urine Examination. By Lawrence Wolff, M. D. Third edition, enlarged by some 300 Essential Formulae, selected from eminent authorities, by Wm. M. Puwell, M. D. (Double number, $1.50 net.) 10. Essentials of Gynecology. By Edwin B. Cragin, M. D. Fifth edition, revised. 11. Essentials of Diseases of the Skin. By Henry W. Stelwagon, M. D. Fourth edition, revised and enlarged. 12. Essentials of Minor Surgery, Bandaging, and Venereal Diseases. By Edward Martin, M. D. Second edition, revised and enlarged. 13. Essentials of Legal Medicine, Toxicology, and Hygiene. This volume is at present out of print. 14. Essentials of Diseases of the Eye. By Edward Jackson, M. D. Third edition, revised and enlarged. 15. Essentials of Diseases of Children. By William M. Powell, M. D. Third edition. 16. Essentials of Examination of Urine. By Lawren< 1 Wolff, M.D. Colored " Vogel Scale." (75 cents net.) 17. Essentials of Diagnosis. By S. Solis-Cohen, M. D., and A. A. ESHNER, M.D. Second edition, thoroughly revised. 18. Essentials of Practice of Pharmacy. By Lucius E. Sayre. Second edition, revised and enlarged. 19. Essentials of Diseases of the Nose and Throat. By E. B. GLEASON, M. D. Third edition, revised and enlarged. 20. Essentials of Bacteriology. By M. V. Ball. M. D. Fourth edition, revised. 21. Essentials of Nervous Diseases and Insanity. By John C. Shaw, M. I). Third edition, revised. 22. Essentials of Medical Physics. By Fred J. Brockway, M.D. Second edition, revised. 23. Essentials of Medical Electricity. By David D. Stewart. M. D., and Edward s. Lawrance, M. J >. 24. Essentials of Diseases of the Ear. By E. B. GLEASON, M.D. Second edition, revised and greatly enlarged. 25. Essentials of Histology. By Louis Leroy, M. D. With 73 original illustrations. Pamphlet containing specimen pages, etc., sent free upon application. Saunders' Medical Hand-Atlases. VOLUMES NOW READY. Atlas and Epitome of Internal Medicine and Clinical Diagnosis. By Dr. Chr. Jakob, of Erlangen. Edited by Augustus A. Eshner, M. D., Professor of Clinical Medicine, Philadelphia Polyclinic. With 179 colored figures on 68 plates, 64 text-illustrations, 259 pages of text. Cloth, S3. 00 net. Atlas of Legal Medicine. By Dr. E. R. von Hofmann, of Vienna. Edited by Frederick Peterson, M. D., Chief of Clinic, Nervous Department, College of Physicians and Surgeons, New York. With 120 colored figures on 56 plates and 193 beautiful half-tone illustrations. Cloth, $3.50 net. Atlas and Epitome of Diseases of the Larynx. By Dr. L. Grunwald, of Munich. Edited by Charles P. Grayson, M. D., Physician-in-Charge, Throat and Nose Department, Hospital of the University of Pennsylvania. With 107 colored figures on 44 plates, 25 text-illustrations, and 103 pages of text. Cloth, $2.50 net. Atlas and Epitome of Operative Surgery. By Dr. O. Zuckerkandl, of Vienna. Edited by J. Chalmers DaCosta, M. D., Professor of Principles of Surgery and Clinical Sur- gery, Jefferson Medical College, Philadelphia. With 24 colored plates, .217 text-illustrations, and 395 pages of text. Cloth, $3.00 net. Atlas and Epitome of Syphilis and the Venereal Diseases. By Prof. Dr. Fraxz Mracek, of Vienna. Edited by L. Bolton Bangs, M. D., Professor of Genito-Urinary Surgery, University and Bellevue Hospital Medical College, New York. With 71 colored plates, 16 illustrations, and 122 pages of text. Cloth, $3.50 net. Atlas and Epitome of External Diseases of the Eye. By Dr. O. Haab, of Zurich. Edited by G. E. de Schweinitz, M. D., Professor of Ophthalmology, Jefferson Medical College, Phila. With 76 colored figures on 40 plates; 228 pages of text. Cloth, S3. 00 net. Atlas and Epitome of Skin Diseases. By Prof. Dr. Franz Mracek, of Vienna. Edited by Henry W. Stf.l- wagon, M. D., Clinical Professor of Dermatology, Jefferson Medical College, Philadelphia. With 63 colored plates, 39 half-tone illustra- tions, and 200 pages of text. Cloth, S3. 50 net. Atlas and Epitome of Special Pathological Histology. By Dr. H. Durck, of Munich. Edited by Ludvig Hektoen, M. D.. Professor of Pathology, Rush Medical College. Chicago. In Two Parts Part I.. in< hiding Circulatory, Respiratory, and Gastro intestinal Tracts, 120 colored figures on 62 plates, 158 pages of text. Part 11., including Liver, Urinary Organs, Sexual < )rgans, Nervous System, Skin, Muscles, and Bones, 123 colored figures on 60 plates, and 192 pages of text. I'd part: Cloth, S3-°° net. 16 Saunders9 Medical Hand-Atlases. VOLUMES JUST ISSUED. Atlas and Epitome of Diseases Caused by Accidents. By Dr. Ed. Golebiewski, of Berlin. Translated and edited with addi- tions by Pearce Bailey, M. D., Attending Physician to the Department of Corrections and to the Almshouse and Incurable Hospitals, New- York. AYith 40 colored plates, 143 text-illustrations, and 600 pages of text. Cloth, §4.00 net. Atlas and Epitome of Gynecology. By Dr. O. Shaeffer, of Heidelberg. From the Second Revised Ger- man Edition. Edited by Richard C. Norris, A. M., M. D., Gyne- cologist to the Methodist Episcopal and the Philadelphia Hospitals ; Surgeon-in-Charge of Preston Retreat, Philadelphia. With 90 colored plates, 65 text-illustrations, and 308 pages of text. Cloth, $3.50 net. Atlas and Epitome of the Nervous System and its Diseases. By Professor Dr. Chr. Jakob, of Erlangen. From the Second Re- vised and Enlarged German Edition. Edited by Edward D. Fisher, M. D., Professor of Diseases of the Nervous System, University and Bellevue Hospital Medical College, New York. With S3 plates and a copious text. Cloth, $3.50 net. Atlas and Epitome of Labor and Operative Obstetrics. By Dr. O. Schaeffer, of Heidelberg. From the Fifth Revised and Enlarged German Edition. Edited by J. Clifton Edgar, M. D., Professor of Obstetrics and Clinical Midwifery, Cornell University Medical School. With 126 colored illustrations. Cloth, $2.00 net. Atlas and Epitome of Obstetric Diagnosis and Treatment. By Dr. O. Schaeffer, of Heidelberg. From the Second Revised and En- larged German Edition. Edited by J. Clifton Edgar, M. D., Professor of Obstetrics and Clinical Midwifery, Cornell University Medical School. 72 colored plates, text-illustrations, and copious text. Cloth, S3. 00 net. Atlas and Epitome of Ophthalmoscopy and Oph- thalmoscopic Diagnosis. By Dr. O. Haab, of Zurich. From the Third Revised and Enlarged German Edition. Edited by (l. E. de Schweinitz, M. D., Professor of Ophthalmology, Jefferson Medical College, Philadelphia. With 152 colored figures and 82 pages of text. Cloth, S300 net. Atlas and Epitome of Bacteriology. Including a Text-Book of Special Bacteriologic Diagnosis. By Prof. Dr. K. B. Lehmann and Dr. R. O. Neumann, of Wurzburg. From the Second devised German Edition. I'M 1 ted by GEORGE H. WEAVER, M. D., Assistant Professor of Pathology and Bacteriology, Push Medical College, Chicago. In Two Parts. Part I., consisting of 632 colored illustra tions on 69 lithographic plates. Part II., consisting of 511 pages of text, illustrated. Per set: Cloth, #5.00 net. ADDITIONAL VOLUMES IN PREPARATION. '7 NOTHNAGEL'S ENCYCLOPEDIA OF PRACTICAL MEDICINE Edited by ALFRED STENGEL, M. D. Professor of Clinical Medicine in the University of Pennsylvania; Visiting Physician to the Pennsylvania Hospital IT is universally acknowledged that the Germans lead the world in Internal .Medicine ; and of all the German works on this subject, Nothnagel's " Ency- clopedia of Special Pathology and Therapeutics" is conceded by scholars to be without question the best System of Medicine in existence. So necessary is this book in the study of Internal Medicine that it comes largely to this country in the original German. In view of these facts, Messrs. W. B. Saunders & Com- pany have arranged with the publishers to issue at once an authorized edition of this great encyclopedia of medicine in English. For the present a set of some ten or twelve volumes, representing the most practical part of this encyclopedia, and selected with especial thought of the needs of the practical physician, will be published. The volumes will contain the real essence of the entire work, and the purchaser will therefore obtain at less than half the cost the cream of the original. Later the special and more strictly scientific volumes will be offered from time to time. The work will be translated by men possessing thorough knowledge of both English and German, and each volume will be edited by a prominent specialist on the subject to which it is devoted. It will thus be brought thoroughly up to date, and the American edition will be more than a mere translation of the Ger- man ; for, in addition to the matter contained in the original, it will represent the very latest views of the leading American specialists in the various departments of Internal Medicine. The whole System will be under the editorial super- vision of Dr. Alfred Stengel, who will select the subjects for the American edition, and will choose the editors of the different volumes. Unlike most encyclopedias, the publication of this work will not be extended over a number of years, but five or six volumes will be issued during the coming year, and the remainder of the series at the same rate. Moreover, each volume will be revised to the date of its publicatfon by the American editor. This will obviate the objection that has heretofore existed to systems published in a number of volumes, since the subscriber will receive the completed work while the earlier volumes are still fresh. The usual method of publishers, when issuing a work of this kind, has been to compel physicians to take the entire System. This seems to us in many cases to be undesirable. Therefore, in purchasing this encyclopedia, physicians will be given the opportunity of subscribing for the entire System at one time ; but any single volume or any number of volumes may be obtained by those who do not desire the complete series. This latter method, while not so profitable to the pub- lisher, offers to the purchaser many advantages which will be appreciated by those who do not care to subscribe for the entire work at one time. This American edition of Nothnagel's Encyclopedia will, without question, form the greatest System of Medicine ever produced, and the publishers feel con- fident that it will meet with general favor in the medical profession. 18 NOTHNAGEL'S ENCYCLOPEDIA VOLUMES JUST ISSUED AND IN PRESS VOLUME I Editor, William Osier, M. D„ F. R. C. P. Professor of Medicine in Johns Hopkins University CONTENTS Typhoid Fever. By Dr. H. Ckrschmann, of Leipsic. Typhus Fever. By Dr. II. Curschmann, of Leipsic. Handsome octavo volume of about 600 pages. Just Issued VOLUME vn Editor, John H. Musser, M. D. Professor 0/ Clinical Medicine, University of Pennsylvania CONTENTS • Diseases of the Bronchi. By Dr. F. A. HOFF- MANN, of Leipsic. Diseases of the Pleura. By Dr. Rosenbach, of Berlin. Pneumonia. By Dr. E. Aufrecht, of Magdeburg. VOLUME II Editor, Sir J. W. Moore, B. A., M.D., F.R.C.P.I., of Dublin Professor of Practice of Medicine, Royal College of Surgeons in Ireland CONTENTS Erysipelas and Erysipeloid. By Dr. H. Len- uartz, of Hamburg. Cholera Asiatica and Cholera Nostras. By Dr. K. von Lieber- meister, of Tiibingen. "Whoooing Cough and Hay Fever. By Dr. G. Sticker, of Giessen. Varicella. By Dr. Tir. von Jur- gensen, of Tiibingen. Variola (including Vaccination). By Dr. H. Immermann, of Basle. Handsome octavo volume of over 700 pages. Just Issued volume vm Editor, Charles G. Stockton, M. D. Professor of Medicine, University of Buffalo CONTENTS Diseasesof the Stomach. By Dr. F. Kiegel, of Giessen. VOLUME IX Editor, Frederick A. Packard, M. D. Physician to the Pennsylvania Hospital and to the Children's Hospital, Philadelphia CONTENTS Diseases of the Liver. By Drs. H.QuiNi ke and G. Hoppe-Seyler, of Kiel. VOLUME in Editor, William P. Northrup, M. D. Professor of Pediatrics, University and Bellevue Medical College CONTENTS Measles, By Dr. Tii. von JCrgensen, of Tubingen. Scarlet Fever. By the same author. Rbtheln. By the same author. VOLUME X Editor, Reginald H. Fitz, A.M., M. D. Hersey Professor of the Theory and Prat :.' •■ of Physic , Ha* vard I University CONTENTS Diseases of the Pancreas. By Dk. L. < 'mi:. of Vienna. Diseases of the Suprarenals. By 1 >R. E. Nl 1 SSER, "i \ ienna. VOLUME VI Editor, Alfred Stengel, M. D. Professor of Clinical Medi, inc. University 0/ Pennsylvania CONTENTS Anemia. By Dr. P. EHRLICH, of Frankfort- on-the-Main, and 1 >r. A. Lazarus, of Char lottenburg. Chlorosis. By Dk. K. von Noorden, of Frankfort-on-the-Main. Dis- eases of the Spleen and Hemorrhagic Diathesis. By Dk. M. I.itti n, of Berlin. VOLUMES IV, V, and XI Editors announced later Vol. IV. — Influenza and Dengue. By Dk. i >. I. Kirn 1 1 \sti rn,oI I lologne. MalarialDis- eases. By Dk. J. Mannaberg, ol Vienna. Vol. \. Tuberculosis and Acute General Miliary Tuberculosis. By Dr.G.< < >km i , of Berlip. Vol. XI Diseases of the Intestines and Peritoneum. By Dr. II. NOTHNAGKL, of Vienna. CLASSIFIED LIST OF THE MEDICAL PUBLICATIONS OF W. B. SAUNDERS Ct COMPANY ANATOMY, EMBRYOLOGY, HISTOLOGY. Bbhm, Davidoff, and Huber— Histology, Clarkson A Text-Book of Histology Haynes - A Manual of Anatomj Heisler —A Text-Book of Embryology Leroy— Essentials of Histology,. . McClellan — Art Anatomy McClellan — Regional Anatomy, . . Nancrede — Essentials of Anat< >m) , . Nancrede— Essentials of Anatomy Manual of Practical Dissection. . and BACTERIOLOGY. Ball — Essentials of Bacteriology Frothingham — Laboratory Guide, . . . . Gorham — Laboratory Bacteriology, . . . Lehmann and Neumann — Atlas of Bacte- riology, Levy and Klemperer's Clinical Bacteri- ology Mallory and Wright— Pathological Tech- nique McFarland — Pathogenic Bacteria CHARTS, DIET-LISTS, ETC. Griffith— Infant's Weight Chart Hart — Diet in Sickness and in Health, . . Keen — Operation Blank, Laine — Temperature Chart Meigs — Feeding in Early Infancy Starr — Diets for Infants and Children, . . Thomas— Diet-Lists CHEMISTRY AND PHYSICS. Brockway Essentials of Medical Physics, Jelliffe and Diekman— Chemistry, '. . . Wolf — Urine Examination Wolff— Essentials of Medical Chemistry, . CHILDREN. American Text-Book Dis.of Children, Griffith— Care of the Baby Griffith —Diseases of Children, . . . Griffith— Infant's Weight Chart, . . . Meigs — Feeding in Early Infancy, . . Powell —Essentials of Diseases of ( 'hildren Starr— Diets for Infants and Children, . DIAGNOSIS. Cohen and Eshner — Essentials of Diag- n sis Corwin — Physical Diagnosis Vierordt — Medical Diagnosis DICTIONARIES. The American Illustrated Medical Dic- tionary The American Pocket Medical Dictionary, Morten — Nurses' Dictionary I 4 EYE, EAR, NOSE, AND THROAT. An American Text-Book of Diseases of the Eye, Ear, Nose, and Throat, .... i De Schweinitz— Diseases of the Eye, . . 6 Friedrich and Curtis— Rhinology, Laryn- gology and Otology, .• 6 Gleason — Essentials of Diseases of the Ear, 15 Gleason— Ess. of Dis. of Nose and Throat, 15 Gradle — Ear, Nose, and Throat 22 Griinwald and Grayson— Atlas of Dis- eases of the Larynx 16 Haab and De Schweinitz — Atlas of Exter- nal Diseases of the Eye 16 Haab and De Schweinitz— Atlas of Oph- thalmoscopy, 17 Jackson — Manual of Diseases of the Eye, 8 Jackson — Essentials of Diseases of Eye, 15 Kyle — Diseases of the Nose and Throat, . 9 GENITO-URINARY. An American Text-Book of Genito-Uri- nary and Skin Diseases 2 Hyde and Montgomery— Syphilis and the Venereal Diseases 8 Martin — Essentials of Minor Surgery, Bandaging, and Venereal Diseases, . . . 15 Mracek and Bangs— Atlas of Syphilis and the Venereal Diseases 16 Saundby — Renal and Urinary Diseases, . . n Senn — Genito-Urinary Tuberculosis, ... 12 Vecki — Sexual Impotence 14 GYNECOLOGY. American Text-Book of Gynecology, Cragin — Essentials of Gynecology, .... 15 Garrigues — Diseases of Women 6 Long — Syllabus of Gynecology, Penrose — Diseases of Women, . Pryor — Pelvic Inflammations, . Schaeffer & Norris — Atlas of Gynecology, 17 HYGIENE. Abbott — Hygiene of Transmissible Diseases 3 Bergey — Principles of Hygiene 4 Pyle — Personal Hygiene 11 MATERIA MEDICA, PHARMACOL- OGY, AND THERAPEUTICS. American Text-Book of Therapeutics, . . 1 Butler — Text-Book of Materia Medica, Therapeutics, and Pharmacology, ... 4 Morris — Ess. of M. M. and Therapeutics, 15 Saunders' Pocket Medical Formulary, . . 12 Sayre — Essentials of Pharmacy 15 Sollmann— Text- Book of Pharmacology, . 12 Stevens — Manual of Therapeutics, ... 13 Stoney — Materia Medica for Nurses, . . 13 Thornton — Prescription-Writing 14 MEDICAL PUBLICATIONS OF IV. B. SAUNDERS & CO. 21 MEDICAL JURISPRUDENCE AND TOXICOLOGY. Chapman — Medical Jurisprudence and Toxicology 5 Golebiewski and Bailey— Atlas of Dis- eases Caused by Accidents 17 Hofmann and Peterson— Atlas of Legal Medicine 16 NERVOUS AND MENTAL DISEASES, ETC. Brower — Manual of Insanity 22 Chapin — Compendium of Insanity, ... 5 Church and Peterson — Nervous and Men- tal Diseases 5 Jakob & Fisher — Atlas of NervousSystem, 17 Shaw — Essentials of Nervous Diseases and Insanity 15 NURSING. Davis — Obstetric and Gynecologic Nursing, 5 Griffith— The Care of the Baby 7 Hart — Diet in Sickness and in Health, . . 7 Meigs — Feeding in Early Infancy 10 Morten — Nurses' Dictionary 10 Stoney — Materia Medica for Nurses, . . 13 Stoney — Practical Points in Nursing, ... 13 Stoney — Surgical Technic for Nurses, . . 13 Watson — Handbook for Nurses 14 OBSTETRICS. An American Text-Book of Obstetrics, . 2 Ashton — Essentials of Obstetrics 15 Boisliniere— Obstetric Accidents, .... 4 Dorland — Modern Obstetrics 6 Hirst — Text-Book of Obstetrics, .... 7 Norris — Syllabus of Obstetrics 10 Schaeffer and Edgar— Atlas of Obstetri- cal Diagnosis and Treatment, 17 PATHOLOGY. An American Text-Book of Pathology, . 2 Diirck and Hektoen— Atlas of Pathologic Histology 16 Kalteyer— Essentials of Pathology, ... 22 Malloryand Wright— Pathological Tech- nique 9 Senn — Pathology and Surgical Treatment of Tumors, 12 Stengel— Text-Book of Pathology, ... 13 Warren— Surgical Pathology and Thera- peutics x4 PHYSIOLOGY. An American Text-Book of Physiology, 2 Budgett— Essentials of Physiology, ... 22 Raymond — Human Physiology 11 Stewart— Manual of Physiology, .... 13 PRACTICE OF MEDICINE. An American Year-Book of Medicine and Sun Anders — Practice of Medicine 4 Eichhorst— Practice of Medicine 6 Lockwood— Manual of the Practice of Medicine 9 Morris— Ess. of Practice of Medicine, . . 15 Salinger and Kalteyer — Modern Medi- cine Il Stevens— Manual of Practice of Medicine, 13 SKIN AND VENEREAL. An American Text-Book of Genito- urinary and Skin Diseases 2 Hyde and Montgomery — Syphilis and the Venereal Diseases 8 Martin — Essentials of Minor Surgery, Bandaging, and Venereal Diseases, . . 15 Mracek and Stelwagon — Atlas of Diseases of the Skin, 16 Stelwagon — Essentials of Diseases of the Skin 15 SURGERY. An American Text-Book of Surgery, . . 2 An American Year-Book of Medicine and Surgery 3 Beck — Fractures 4 Beck — Manual of Surgical Asepsis, ... 4 Da Costa — Manual of Surgery 5 International Text-Book of Surgery, . . 8 Keen — Operation Blank 8 Keen — The Surgical Complications and Sequels of Typhoid Fever 8 Macdonald — Surgical Diagnosis and Treat- ment 9 Martin— Essentials of Minor Surgery, Bandaging, and Venereal Diseases, . . 15 Martin— Essentials of Surgery 15 Moore — Orthopedic Surgery 10 Nancrede — Principles of Surgery 10 Pye — Bandaging and Surgical Dressing, . 11 Scudder — Treatment of Fractures, ... 12 Senn — Genito-Urinary Tuberculosis, ... 12 Senn — Practical Surgery, 12 Senn — Syllabus of Surgery 12 Senn — Pathology and Surgical Treatment of Tumors 12 Warren — Surgical Pathology and Thera- peutics *4 Zuckerkandl and Da Costa— Atlas of Operative Surgery 16 URINE AND URINARY DISEASES. Ogden — Clinical Examination of the Urine, 11 Saundby -Renal and Urinary Diseases, . n Wolf - Handbook of Urine- Examina- tion 14 Wolff — Essentials of Examination of Urine *5 MISCELLANEOUS. Bastin — Laboratory Exercises in Botany, . 4 Golebiewski and Bailey- Atlas of Dis- e tses Caused by Accidents 17 Gould and Pyle — Anomalies and Curiosi- ties of Medicine 7 Grafstrom— Massage 7 Keating— 1 low to Examine for Life Insur- ance . . Saunders' Medical Hand-Atlases, . . 16,17 Saunders' Pocket Medical Formulary, . . 12 Saunderb' Question-Compends, . . .14,15 Stewart and Lawrance— Essentials of Medical Electricity 15 Thornton — Dose-Book and Manual of Prescription-Writing 13 Warwick and Tunstall First Aid to the Injured and Sick 14 Books in Preparation. Jelliffe arid Diekman's Chemistry. A Text-Book of Chemistry. By Smith Ely Jelliffe, M. D., Ph.D., Professor of Pharmacology, College of Pharmacy, New York ; and George C. Diekman, Ph. (J., M. 1)., Professor of Theoretical and Applied Pharmacy, College of Pharmacy, New York. Octavo, 550 pages, illustrated. Brower's Manual of Insanity. A Practical Manual of Insanity. By Daniel R. Brower, M. D., Pro- fessor of Nervous and Mental Diseases, Rush Medical College, Chicago. 121110 volume of 425 pages, illustrated. Kalteyer's Pathology. Essentials of Pathology. By F. J. Kalteyer, M. D., Assistant Demon- strator of Clinical Medicine, Jefferson Medical College ; Pathologist to the Lying-in Charity Hospital ; Assistant Pathologist to the Philadel- phia Hospital. A New Volume in Saunders' Question- Compend Series. Gradle on the Nose, Throat, arid Ear. Diseases of the Nose, Throat, and Ear. By Henry Gradle, M. D., Professor of Ophthalmology and Otology, Northwestern University Medical School, Chicago. Octavo, 800 pages, illustrated. Budgett's Physiology. Essentials of Physiology. By Sidney P. Budgett, M. D., Professor of Physiology, Washington University, St. Louis, Mo. A New Volume in Saunders' Question- Compend Series. Griffith's Diseases of Children. A Text-Book of the Diseases of Children. By J. P. Crozer Griffith, Clinical Professor of Diseases of Children, University of Pennsylvania. Galbraith on the Four Epochs of Woman's Life. The Four Epochs of Woman's Life: A Study in Hygiene. By Anna M. Galbraith, M. I)., Fellow New York Academy of Medicine; At- tending Physician Neurologic Department New York Orthopedic Hos- pital and Dispensary, etc. With an Introduction by John H. Musser, M. 1)., Professor of Clinical Medicine, University of Pennsylvania. 1 21110 volume of about 200 pages. Date Due .HINTED IN U.S. A CAT NO. 24 161 D 000 224 608 o CO == a? k 11859a 1900 v. 2 Hove 11, William II An American text-book of physic-lory MEDICAL SCIENCES LIBRARY UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA, IRVINE IRVINE, CALIFORNIA 92664 ■ • ■ •