BIOLOGICAL BULLETIN OF THE fiDarine Biological Xaboraton? WOODS HOLE, MASS. EMtorial Staff E. G. CONKLIN — Princeton University. GEORGE T. MOORE — The Missoiiri Botanic Garden. T. H. MORGAN — Columbia University. W. M. WHEELER — Harvard University. E. B. WILSON — Columbia University. /IDanacjincj FRANK R. LILLIE — The University of Chicago. VOLUME XXXVIII. WOODS HOLE, MASS. JANUARY TO JUNE, 1920 PRESS OF THE NEW ERA PRINTING COMPANY i_ANC«STER, PA. CONTENTS OF VOLUME XXXVIII. No. i. JANUARY, 1920. REIGHARD, JACOB. The Breeding Behavior of the Suckers and Minnows I STRONG, LEONELL C. Roughoid, A Mutant Located to the Left of Sepia in the Chromosome of Drosophila melano- gaster 33 No. 2. FEBRUARY, 1920. HESS, WALTER N. Notes on the Biology of Some Common LampyridcB 39 DAWSON, A. B. An Exception to Bateson's Rule of Secondary Symmetry 77 SHAFFER, E. L. A Comparative Study of the Chromosomes of Lachnosterna (Coleoptera) 83 No. 3. MARCH, 1920. ROBBINS, H. L. AND CHILD, C. M. Carbon Dioxide Produc- tion in Relation to Regeneration in Planaria dorotocephala 103 BALDWIN, FRANCIS M. Susceptible and Resistant Phases of the Dividing Sea-Urchin Egg when subjected to Various Concentrations of Lipoid- Soluble Substances, especially the Higher Alcohols 123 BERRY, S. STILLMAN. Light Production in Cephalopods, I . . 141 No. 4. APRIL, 1920. BERRY, S. STILLMAN. Light Production in Cephalopods, II. 171 GOODRICH, H. B. Rapidity of Activation in the Fertilization of Nereis 1 96 DANCHAKOFF, VERA. Immunity and the Power of Digestion. 202 HARMAN, MARY T. Chromosome Studies in Tettigidae. II. Chromosomes of Paratettix BB and CC and their Hybrid BC 213 iii IV CONTENTS BRIDGES, CALVIN B. White-Ocelli — An Example of a "Slight" Mutant Character with Normal Viability 231 ISHII, O. Observations on the Sexual Cycle of the Guinea Pig. 237 No. 5. MAY, 1920. JONES, D. F. Selective Fertilization in Pollen Mixtures . ... 251 WODSEDALEK, J. E. Studies on the Cells of Cattle with Special Reference to S per mato genesis, Oogonia, and Sex-Deter- mination 290 HEILBRUNN, L. V. Studies in Artificial Parthenogenesis. III. Cortical Change and the Initiation of Maturation in the Egg of Cumingia 317 HAUSMAN, LEON AUGUSTUS. A Contribution to the Life History of Amoeba Proteus Leidy 340 No. 6. JUNE, 1920. HYMAN, LIBBIE H. The Axial Gradients in Hydrozoa, III. Experiments on the Gradient of Tubidaria 353 SHAFFER, ELMER L. The Germ-Cells of Cicada (Tibicen} septemdecim (Homoptera) 404 Vol. XXXVIII. January, 1920. No. i. BIOLOGICAL BULLETIN THE BREEDING BEHAVIOR OF THE SUCKERS AND MINNOWS. I. THE SUCKERS.1 • JACOB REIGHARD. CONTENTS. I. Introduction to the Series i II. Breeding Behavior of the Suckers 3 A. The White Sucker 3 1. The Breeding Grounds and Breeding Season : . 3 2. General Activities of Breeding Fish . 4 3. Coloration and Color Changes 6 4. Sexual Differences 6 Color 6 Pearl Organs 7 Fin Length 8 5. Breeding Activities 9 B. The Common Red-horse 15 1. Breeding Grounds and Breeding Season 15 2. Sexual Differences 16 3. Coloration 18 4. Breeding Activities 19 C. The Hogsucker 20 1. General Activities 20 2. Sexual Differences 21 3. Breeding Activities 22 III. Summary of Observations. A . The White Sucker .... 24 B. The Red-horse 26 C. The Hogsucker 28 IV. Conclusions 29 V. Literature 31 i. INTRODUCTION TO THE SERIES. This paper is the first of a series on the breeding habits of nine species of suckers and minnows. These fishes form a well- defined group by some systematists united into a single family, 1 Contributions from the Zoological Laboratory of the University of Michigan. I 2 JACOB REIGHARD. by others separated into the Catostomidse and Cyprinidae. To accumulate the field notes and sketches on which the papers are based has been the work of many seasons. If there were no bad weather, no university duties, and no human interference with breeding environment or breeding fish, such work might be carried on with as little interruption as that of the laboratory; but it would not progress as rapidly, for observations need to be many times repeated. The behavior is often so complicated or so rapid that it is only by analyzing it into its elements and observing -each of these repeatedly that a degree of certainty is possible. Such attitudes as these shown in Fig. 3 of this paper may be correctly represented only after many observations on the position of each fin and on every other detail. The observa- tions upon which my descriptions are based have been very many for each element of the behavior, often in the neighborhood of a hundred, sometimes probably several hundred, or even thousand. Although the work has been spread over a period of years no one should suppose that the record is complete. I have studied only the breeding behavior and that in but few fishes; I have not the life history or the full natural history of any one. Of the breeding behavior I shall try to give for each species a composite picture taken from many fish through many seasons. This I give in such detail as I have, because that seems necessary to clearness. It further affords a better basis for the discussion of theories to be considered in the final papers of the series. The behavior that I describe may be easily seen in suitable places and at the proper season. Yet few are likely to take the trouble. This has seemed to me an added reason for fullness of treatment. The suckers and minnows that I have studied, with the ex- ception of the blunt-nosed minnow, Pimephales notatus (Rafin- esque) breed .in running water. During the breeding season the males of all the species studied have pearl organs, hard, tough, white, usually conical elevations of the skin which consist of cornified epidermal cells. They occur in many situations. Some of them are so small as to be visible only under a lens; others may be seen with the naked eye at a distance of ten or twelve feet. They commonly make the surface of the breeding males distinctly rough to the touch and are in that case referred to as "effective" pearl organs. BREEDING BEHAVIOR OF SUCKERS AND MINNOWS. 3 The drawings that show fish in action have been made from field notes and sketches with the help of specimens and in some cases with the aid of models or photographs. I am indebted to Mr. Charles R. Knight for valuable suggestions in connection with the drawings but he is in no way responsible for their obvious shortcomings. To my former student, Professor Norman H. Stewart, of Bucknell University, I am indebted for data from his unpublished manuscript on the distribution of the pearl organs. To the United States Commissioner of Fisheries, Doctor Hugh M. Smith, I am indebted for permission to publish. 2. BREEDING BEHAVIOR OF THE SUCKERS. A. The While Sucker (Catostomus commersonii LeSueur). i. The Breeding Grounds and Breeding Season. — A little way from the State Bass Hatchery, near Grand Rapids, Michigan, Mill Creek is spanned by a bridge of the Pere Marquette Railway. Just below the bridge, where the stream is three or four rods wide, a line of four-inch waterpipe is laid across its bed. An eighth of a mile above the bridge the stream is dammed to furnish water for the hatchery. Between the dam and the pipe-line it is made up of a series of alternating rapids and pools. For a little distance above the bridge and beneath it the water runs six to twelve inches deep, swift and smooth. Below the pipe-line it breaks into ripples. Under the smoother water the bottom is sandy gravel, but under the broken water this gives place to larger stones. Over the gravel in the smoother water a thin brown mantle of silt and algal growth usually stretches without interruption from shore to shore and obscures the bottom. Rapids of this sort are typical of a drift-covered country and afford the characteristic breeding ground of the white sucker.1 In the spring patches of the gravel bottom in the upper water of the rapids often look as though they had been scoured by a broom. The silt mantle is absent from these patches and the bright colors of the clean gravel and sand throw them into 1 The usual name in Southern Michigan; known also as common sucker, fine- scaled sucker, brook sucker. I am indebted to Professor W. M. Smallwood for permission to use his un- published notes on this species. He records seeing it in the spawning attitude on stony bottom in Lake Clear in the Adirondacks in late June (cf. Reighard, 1915)- 4 JACOB REIGHARD. sharp contrast with the surrounding silt-covered bottom. The patches show where the suckers have been breeding. The water of a rapid is rarely so smooth that one can see readily into it. Not often is such a rapid near a bridge or other perch; but under this Mill Creek bridge the surface of the stream is but little broken and on a sunny, cloudless day, when the water is clear and there is no wind to ruffle iits surface, it is possible to see in detail what happens on the rapid. With field glasses the fish may be studied almost as readily as though they wrere in air. From this vantage point I watched the white suckers at intervals.1 My work was done by day. It is well known that in the spring suckers ascend small streams in great numbers at night and it is possible that their breeding activities are continued at night. They are often interrupted by colder weather or roily water. 2. General Activities of Breeding Fish. — About 2 o'clock on April 23, 1903, I cautiously took my place on the Mill Creek railroad bridge. Numerous white suckers were on the rapids. Although I walked with extreme slowness and made no sudden movements of any part of my body the fish were at once aware of my coming and scurried to the shelter of the banks and nearer pools. I sat quiet and in the course of fifteen minutes they began to reappear in the shallow, swift water. Thereafter, for an hour, any quick movement on my part resulted in the fish starting swiftly up stream, but if the movement was not repeated they dropped slowly down-stream to where they had been. To get the field glasses to the eyes or the hand to the notebook without startling the fish needed a movement so slow that it must have been scarcely perceptible at the distance of twenty- five or thirty feet at which the fish were. It was probably about two inches per second. As time went on the fish became grad- ually used to my presence and after an hour were no longer dis- turbed by slow movements. By three o'clock twenty suckers from eight to twelve inches long were on the rapid and were moving slowly up stream in small groups. The fish stopped 1 From April 23 to May 6, 1903. In Honey Creek near Ann Arbor, I saw them breeding April 27 to May 2, and in Mallet Creek, Ann Arbor, on May 10, 1909. The two creeks last named are only about a third the width of Mill Creek (Grand Rapids) at the point at which the suckers were seen. BREEDING BEHAVIOR OF SUCKERS AXU MINNOWS. 5 here an'd there in the rapid on the patches of cleaned gravel and were seen to take gravel into the mouth and spit it out. They were presumably in quest of eggs that had been laid in the disturbed gravel areas and their occurrence in small groups is perhaps in part the result of the distribution of such areas. In their search they were accompanied by numerous small minnows doubtless on the same quest. Other suckers were seen crossing the pipe-line. At four o'clock no suckers were to be seen from the bridge, but a dozen were found just below the dam an eighth of a mile further up stream. The fish had appar- ently covered this distance in an hour. At nine o'clock the same evening the search light showed suckers still crossing the pipe line bound up stream. In my experience the white sucker is one of the most difficult of our native fishes to approach in the open. Ordinarily it becomes accustomed to the observer with extreme slowness and at no time permits him any great freedom of movement. Con- fined with other fish in an aquarium it is among the last to become accustomed to the observer or to take food.1 1 I have noted but two exceptions to this general fact, (i) On May 6, in the morning, I placed in an outdoor aquarium at the Mill Creek Hatchery four males and two females that had been captured in a seine on the previous evening. At three o'clock on the same day the fish were moving about and feeding and by four o'clock they were spawning. They did not react to an observer within two or three feet of them. (2) At Douglas Lake in Cheboygan County, Michigan, suckers are found in rather deep water. At night they come into shallower water to feed and are occasionally seen there at dawn. At such times they flee to the deeper water at the first glimpse of a moving object. lu late June the log perch (Percina caprodes) are laying their eggs in the sand in very shallow water. At that time suckers enter shallow water in the day time and feed on the eggs of the log perch. Each sucker is accompanied by a group of log perch which appear to be feeding on eggs uncovered by him and perhaps on other crumbs from his table. At this time the suckers may be approached with little trouble and I have come close enough to photograph them as they lay at my feet. I have thought this absence of the suckers' usual wariness due to the presence of the log perch. These are breeding and are then unafraid. In deeper water the sucker has probably found freedom from disturbance where they were present. Safety and log perch have been closely linked in his experience. So now, so long as the log perch are on the shallows, he is not easily startled and feeds there undisturbed by sights that would otherwise send him hurrying to shelter. To these observations may be added one of Smallwood (unpublished notes). At the end of June he found Catostomus commersonii and two other species of sucker on stonv bottom in the shallow water of Lake Clear in the Adirondacks. 6 JACOB REIGHARD. 3. Coloration and Color Changes. — The mature white sucker, when seen in its native waters or in captivity, is ordinarily uni- formly olivaceous on the back and sides and white below. There is no color pattern nor are there color differences between the sexes. The suckers seen from the bridge (Fig. 4) were so different in coloration from all that I had seen before, that I was at first doubtful as to their identity. Each had a broad yellow-white stripe which crossed the occiput and extended thence down the sides. When some of these fish were seined they were found to have the usual uniformly olivaceous color. They were placed in an aquarium, males and females together, and four hours later the males had begun to move about and to feed. Shortly afterward the light stripe appeared across the occiput and down the sides. Beneath the light stripe was a broad dark stripe (Fig. 4) and in one of the males this had a rosy tinge. During the actual pairing described below, the rosy tinge gave place to a brilliant crimson. Later I often saw the light stripe appear in a few seconds on uniformly colored males that were on the rapids. This happens regularly in the breeding season when the sexes are together. 4. Sexual Differences. — It is at first difficult to discriminate between males and females. As seen from the bridge the paired fins are transparent white in both and in both the yellowish white stripe crosses the occiput and extends down the sides. But the males are on the average smaller than the females and slenderer. It is soon apparent that the occipito-lateral stripe is whiter in them and that their backs may be flecked with white especially between the dorsal fin and the occiput (Fig. 4, male at right of female). In the region of the occiput the white flecks may form a distinct patch which, seen from a distance, looks like fungus. The white flecks are perhaps not always present in males, but I have never seen them in females. In pairing males, seen in the aquarium, the dorsal half of the eye is lighter colored than in females, but I do not know that this is not the case at other times. The differences so far noted afford C. commersonii and one of the other species, probably C. catosiomus, were seen in the spawning attitude. The fish were not disturbed when a boat, in which were • two children and a barking dog, was poled about above them so that they were not more than five feet from it. BREEDING BEHAVIOR OF SUCKERS AND MINNOWS. 7 excellent field characters by which the sexes of the breeding fish may be distinguished in their native waters. Less striking differential characters are the greater length of the caudal and lower fins of the male, the difference in length of his caudal lobes and his possession of pearl organs. Effective pearl organs (vide introduction) occur on the male in the following situations (Fig. i): (i) Large, sharp-pointed FIG. i. Lateral view of a part of a male and female of Catostomus commersomi drawn to the same scale. The black dots represent pearl organs. The anal fin and lower lobe of the caudal are longer in the male and bear large pearl organs . All the scales of his sides bear small pearls, somewhat larger behind the caudal The female has no pearl organs. organs are found on the anal fin and on the lower part of the caudal. They may be visible to the unaided eye at a distance of three or four yards. (2) The caudal margins of the scales on the sides bear small hemispherical organs, which are effective behind the dorsal. (3) The upper surfaces of the pectorals and both surfaces of the pelvic fins bear small organs. (4) The rays of the dorsal fin bear small organs. Pearl organs do not occur in the female (Fig. i). 8 JACOB REIGHARD. The difference between the sexes in fin length are shown in Table I. TABLE I. SHOWING IN MILLIMETERS THE AVERAGE LENGTH OF FINS IN MALES AND FEMALES OF Catostomus commersonii OF EQUAL LENGTH, THE DIFFERENCE IN AVERAGE LENGTH OF FINS AND THE PERCENTAGE DIFFERENCE. Length, Tip Cauda! of Snout to Lower Anal. Dorsal, Pelvic, Pec- Base of Caudal. Lobe, Length. Length, Length. Length, toral, Length. Males, M ^12 62 6s. S 47.8 42 1:7.1; Females, F "?I2 S4.6 40-8 4S-7 -27.7 ci Difference, D == M - F OOO + 7-4 + 15-7 + 2.1 + 4-3 + 6.5 Percentage of Difference, % D = (M — F)-F. ooo + 1^.4 + 71. c; + c o H-II d + 17.8 (+31- 1) By fin length is meant the greatest distance from base of fin to its margin, approximately the length of the longest fin ray. The table is based on nine fish of which six were males. The males ranged in length from 260 to 405 mm. with an average of 312 mm. All had wrell developed pearls. The females aver- aged 263 mm. in length (210, 215, 365). In order to compare fish of equal length the female average length has been made equal to that of the male and the average fin lengths of the females as obtained from measurements have been corrected in proportion. The figures in the lower horizontal line therefore show in percentages the sex difference in length of fins in fish of the same length. Since the caudals of two of the females were broken there is added for the caudal a corrected percentage (31.1) obtained by comparison of a single male of 340 mm. length with a female of 365 mm. From the table it appears that in fish of equal length the anal and caudal (lower lobe) of the male are about 31 per cent, longer than those of the female; the pectoral about 18 per cent, longer; the pelvic about n per cent, and the dorsal only 5 per cent. In the female the caudal lobes are of about equal length whereas in the male the lower lobe in two perfect specimens averaged about 10 per cent, longer than the upper. The fins appear to differ in robustness in about the same proportion as in length. This is shown by the width at mid-length of the longest anal ray in a male and female of equal length; in the male 4.5 mm. in the BREEDING BEHAVIOR OF SUCKERS AND MINNOWS. 9 female 2.6 mm. In smaller individuals there is no great differ- ence between the length of fins in individuals of opposite sexes, but these differences appear with increasing size. A male of 135 mm. when compared to a female of 137 mm., had slightly shorter dorsal, caudal and pectorals, but somewhat longer anals and pelvics. It is to be noted that in adult specimens those fins of the male (lower caudal lobe, anal) that bear the largest pearl organs also exceed the corresponding fins of the female by the largest percentage. (Cf. Table I. and Fig. I.) Indeed the fins of the male may be divided into three groups on the basis of the per- centage by which they exceed the corresponding fins of the female; the anal and caudal with 31 per cent, excess; the pec- toral and pelvic with 10 to 20 per cent, and the dorsal with 5 per cent. The size of the pearls borne on the fins of these three groups is roughly proportional to these percentages. When the fish are seen close at hand, or under favorable conditions with field glasses, the pearl organs of the male and the greater size of his anal fin suffice to distinguish him from the female (Fig. i). But were the sexes identical in form, size and color, the behavior differences described in another place would differentiate them. 5. Breeding Activities. — During the. breeding season males are at all times much more numerous on the rapids than females and during the greater part of the time none but males are present. It does not follow that males are actually more num- erous than females. On the contrary data that I have collected at other localities and at times when the fish were not breeding indicate that the males and females are equally numerous. (Reighard, 1915.) In the breeding season the females do not mingle with the males on the rapids until ready to lay their eggs. But from time to time a female comes from her retreat in the deeper water above or below the rapid or from beneath the bank and takes her place on the rapid. If no males happen to be near she may lie quiet in one place for a considerable time. But if males are near they at once approach her, sometimes one or two, sometimes as many as ten. Pairing is best seen when but two males are involved and will be first described under these 10 JACOB REIGHARD. conditions. As the males approach the resting female she hurries forward as though to escape, but presently stops with her belly. on the bottom. As the males again approach she hur- ries forward a second time, but soon stops as before. .Thus she appears to be driven here and there over the spawning ground, too "coy" to allow the males near her. After a varying number of apparent efforts to escape her "coyness" vanishes and she rests quietly on the bottom and permits the males to come near. In what follows I describe the spawning behavior as I saw it very many times from the bridge or from the banks of Mill Creek. Details were observed several times in the aquarium (Fig. 4). When a male comes within a few inches of the waiting female he is often seen to stop, spread his pectorals, erect his dorsal and protrude his jaws (Fig. 4, second fish from bottom). Then, for perhaps a second, his head trembles with a slight, rapid vibration from side to side. The movement is not unlike the tremor of a palsied hand. It is like the tremor that one may produce in- his own head by strong continuous contraction of the muscles of the neck. This tremor may be seen not only when a male approaches a female, but often when he approaches another male on the spawning ground. I have never seen it in a female. FIG. 2. A female of Catostomus commersonii engaged in pairing, with a male on each side of her. The body and tail of one male are shaded. The pearl organs are shown on the anal and caudal of the nearer male, but not elsewhere. The figure represents the pairing act near its end with the head of the female well above the bottom, which is represented by the irregular horizontal line. Drawn with the help of a photograph (see Fig. 4). It is probably nothing more than a beginning of the tremor of the whole body which accompanies spawning. It necessarily produces a vibration in the water which may be of such a rate as to stimulate other fish through the skin, ear or lateral line BREEDING BEHAVIOR OF SUCKERS AND MINNOWS. II organs (Parker, 1917). In many fishes the spreading of the dorsal at the breeding season results in the display of a con- spicuous marking or color pattern (Reeves, 1907 and citations). This is true of some of the minnows to be described in later num- bers of this series. //• is noteworthy that in the suckers, although the dorsals are unmarked, the display movement (Fig. 2) does not differ from that of those fishes that have a conspicuous dorsal. As the male approaches the female another change becomes evident. The dark stripe on his side (Fig. 4), which may have become rosy a little while before, now suddenly turns to a vivid scarlet and remains so during the pairing. At the same time the eye becomes red and continues so while the female is present. The males with fully extended pectorals and erected dorsals now press close against the female one on either side (Figs. 2,3). FIG. 3. Diagram showing the pairing of Catostomus commersonii as seen from the dorsal side. Compare Fig. 2. The smallest males that I have seen pairing were about six inches long. The two are usually of the same size and commonly are a good deal smaller than the female. But they are not always of the same size and one or both may be as large as the female. When the males are in position (Figs. 2, 3) their adjacent pectorals are spread beneath the female. Their caudal fins press on either side against that of the female, but may extend for some distance behind and below it, so that their distal parts press against each other. Their anals are spread and extended 12 JACOB REIGHARD. downward so that they press against the sides of the tail and sometimes against the anal of the female. The backs of the males are arched and their dorsal fin-rays, spread like the ribs of an extended fan, stretch the membrane between them (Figs. 2,3)- The lateral surfaces of the anal and caudal and the sides behind the dorsal, all of which are roughened by pearl organs (Fig. i) are thus pressed vise-like against the female so that she is firmly held (Figs. 2, 3). But the roughened caudals and anals of the two males also press against each other where no part of the female separates them. Thus the pearl organs aid males to keep their positions with reference to the female as well as to each other. When the fish have come into position there is a rapid vibration of the whole bodies of all three together. This is wide and vigorous behind the dorsal fin, while in the region of the head it is a little more than a tremor. At this time the fish are often in water so shallow that their backs are exposed. The powerful movement of the tails of the three fish stirs up the gravel and a cloud of sand is released and washed downstream. At this time, in the aquarium, one may see milt spurt from the genital openings of both males and cloud the water. No doubt the eggs are extruded at the same instant and buried in the gravel, but the water is made so turbid by sand and milt that I have not seen the eggs laid. I have estimated the length of the spawning act at a second and a half but have not actually timed it. It is often repeated especially by the larger males. Large males are often taken in which the front edge of the anal fin is raw and \vorn by rubbing against the gravel. I have seen nothing of the sort in females, but their anal fins are smaller than those of males and are protected by them during the spawning act, while the number of pairings of the average female can be but half that of the average male. When a pairing act is completed the female moves on, usually upstream, and presently pairs with other males on another part of the breeding ground. Her eggs are thus scattered in small lots over a considerable area, very likely over more than one rapid and are commonly fertilized by many pairs of males. The two BREEDING BEHAVIOR OF SUCKERS AND MINNOWS. 13 males separate, the red stripe on their sides and the red in their eyes fades, but the white occipito-lateral stripe remains for some time. Each male now moves about and feeds as before. And so they continue until another female appears when one or both may succeed in pairing with her and this may happen on any part of the spawning ground. Thus the eggs fertilized by one male may lie anywhere in the gravel of a rapid or in that of several rapids. The breeding activities are in no way centered about individual males, for the cooperation of two males in pairing makes it impossible to know what eggs are fertilized by an individual male. I have never collected from the bottom the eggs laid at a single pairing. But the smaller fish lurking in the neighborhood tell one plainly enough where they are. The black-nosed dace (Rhinichthys atronasus) and the rainbow darter (Etheostoma coeruleum) gather at once in great numbers over the spot where the pairing suckers were. They come in a straight line from down stream attracted, no doubt, by the trail of milt, eggs or bottom materials swept down by the current. They gather in an area six or eight inches across and each burrows in the bottom with its snout as though seeking eggs. The whole little area is soon concealed by their wriggling tails, close-set like threads in the pile of velvet. Some of the eggs may have been swept down stream, but many of them must be buried where the small fish are rooting. When more than two males follow a female (Fig. 4) it may be difficult to see what happens. When she finally stops the two males nearest or most vigorous in the pursuit attempt to pair with her. But the others at once crowd about and try to force their way between her and her mates. They try either to squeeze in at the sides of the female from above so as to force her mates outward, or to wedge themselves beneath the pairing males from the side so as to force them up and take their places. But once the two males have the female firmly held between them it is difficult to dispossess either and I have never seen this happen. As many as ten males have been seen with a single female during the spawning act, and the act was nevertheless completed; but often, when many crowd about, she interrupts JACOB REIGHARD. BREEDING BEHAVIOR OF SUCKERS AND MINNOWS. 15 the pairing and moves upstream as though seeking an oppor- tunity to pair unmolested. This she may secure and the pairing then takes place in the manner already described. Although supernumerary males may be present and may attempt to inter- fere they take no part in the normal pairing. While they crowd about the female and attempt to gain a place at her side, there are no combats between them. When not at the side of the female they seem to pay no attention to one another. When the breeding season is over the male suckers lose their pearl organs. They were beginning to shed them in southern Michigan on May 13, 1913. Both sexes become uniformly olivaceous on sides and back. They are no longer commonly seen on their breeding grounds, and in Michigan most of them seek deeper waters.1 B. The Common Red-Horse (Moxostoma aureolum'Le Sueur). i. Breeding Grounds and Breeding Season. — In southern Michigan the red-horse breeds in May. My two dates are May 17, 1904, and May 4, 1905. On these dates the breeding season of the white sucker was nearing its end. While the breed- ing grounds of the two species are the quieter, upper parts of rapids in shallow water with gravel bottom and, while the two species often breed in the same rapids, I have not found the red-horse in streams as small as those sometimes frequented by the breeding white sucker. The sucker may spawn in brooks so small that one may step across them but I have found the 1 In Walnut Lake Hankinson (1908) reports the species in water 15-40 ft. in depth in summer and a few were found in a restricted part of the lake in water 80 ft- in depth in April and May. I have found it in Douglas Lake, Cheboygan County, Mich., from July to September in water up to 45 ft. in depth (Reighard, 1915). Smallwood (unpublished notes) reports the return of suckers to Lake Clear after breeding in Sucker Brook. On the other hand Forbes and Richardson (1908) say: " It is with us essentially a species ot creeks and smaller rivers, nearly four times as common, according to our data in the former as in the latter. . . . Our col- lections show that it is much more likely to be abundant on bottoms with more or less rock and sand than on a completely muddy bottom and that it has also a decided preference for clear, swift water." Without a knowledge of the dates at which Forbes and Richardson's collections were made or of the size of the fish taken it is not possible to say to what extent adult suckers collected by them occur on the rapids at other than the breeding season. Certain it is that they are abun- dant in the deeper water of some inland lakes of Michigan and in the Great Lakes when not breeding. i6 JACOB REIGHARD. breeding red-horse in streams not less than thirty or forty feet in width. 2. Sexual Differences. — There are no known color characters by which the sexes of the red-horse may be distinguished with certainty at any season but there are structural differences. According to Forbes and Richardson (1908) the lower fins are "longest in the male." Table II. has been made in the same way as Table I. (See p. 8.) The averages were obtained from five males and two females, all breeding fish. It shows that all the fins of the male are longer than those of a female of the same length with the possible exception of the caudal. The caudal appears to be 5 per cent, shorter in the male. But since some of the caudals are imperfect a comparison was made of a single perfect male of 205 mm. with a perfect female of 280 mm. on the basis of equal length. This shows that the upper lobe of the caudal is 12 per cent, longer in the male and the lower lobe 33 per cent, longer. The latter value is included in parentheses in the table. The anal and pectoral are longer in the male by about 15 per cent. The dorsals and pelvics are longer by about 10 per cent. The lower lobe of the caudal of the male is not only longer than that of the female, but about 14 per cent, longer than the upper lobe. In the female the two lobes are of equal length. TABLE II. SHOWING ix MILLIMETERS THE AVERAGE LENGTH OF FINS IN MALES AND FEMALES OF Moxostoma aureolum OF EQUAL LENGTH, THE DIFFERENCE IN AVERAGE LENGTH OF FINS AND THE PERCENTAGE DIFFERENCE. Length, Tip Caudal of Snout to Lower Anal, Dorsal. Pelvic, Pect- Base of Cau- dal. Mm. Lobe. Length. Length. Length. Length. oral, Length. Average for s males, M 2T.Q ^6.8 SO. 4 47 T.6.2 4Q.2 Average for 2 females, F 2?O 60 44 4.3 T. 3 •21. C Difference, D = M — F .... OOO — 3.2 + 6.4 + 4 + 3-2 + 7-7 Percentage of Difference, % D = (M - F) : F. . OOO q.7 + 14 c 4-Q7 + 04 + IS-8 (+33) The breeding males are further distinguished by the possession of conspicious pearl organs. (Figs. 5, 6.) On the end of the snout and sides of the head as far as the caudal margin of the preoperculum are numerous large sharp-pointed organs, more BREEDING BEHAVIOR OF SUCKERS AND MINNOWS. f^' M? QI en c< S4 & '— b "« rt .2 u M •s x ^ en e 8 0 'a » a rt y bJO — ^ O u rt -~ Q> ^ = en '^3 c o « H o^o £ CC as.. . ~ - .2f bii -3 d, S c "S w rt i; a 3 a — •'• rt 4-) G, § 1 £ O T? e u o g ^: * S 5 -a ^ ..3 O « o « 1) _x -— - £J H •= c o ° in ^ CJ nj "rt g S d " TJ o rt ^ rt •o •— ^ o ? t ^ S ^ n Q X •g -a s ~ d rt «i <-> rt o E t3 o C -*-> £U C '-*— ' --— O 4J en c /• "^ M rs 3 s r: -- 1 rt ' fc S ~ i SI'S & 1 8 JACOB REIGHARD. than two hundred in a well-developed specimen. On the sides of the anal and caudal fins and on the tail for a little distance in ' front of the caudal fin are large disc-shaped organs (not pointed). Smaller, pointed organs occur on all the scales of the sides and back, on the dorsal, on the upper surface of the pectoral and on both surfaces of the pelvic. All these organs are effective in proportion to their size. Here again the caudal and anal fins of the male, which exceed those of the female by the largest percentage, bear the largest and most effective pearl organs. It is probable that further data would make possible a more precise statement of the relation between size of pearls and relative size • of fins. This species is one of the few in which pearl organs have been noted in the female. In a single specimen I have found minute organs on the top of the head and on the first few scales of the back behind the head. They were especially numerous about the upper end of the opercular opening. They were too small to be effective and care was needed to see them at all. 3. Coloration. — At 2:30 P.M. on May 17, 1904, I found ten to twrelve red-horse lying quiet in shallow water near the bank of Mill Creek, near Ann Arbor. The water was smooth and I was able to come within ten feet of them and watch them with field glasses from an elevated position. The fish were from twelve to fourteen inches long from tip of snout to tip of tail. The red-horse, seen at other seasons, whether in its native waters or in aquaria, has the sides and back in both sexes uni- formly olivaceous, but somewhat darker above. The belly is smoky white. The sides show tinges of salmon in front of the dorsals and the lower fins have some orange near the base. The red-horse before me were of such exceptional coloration that they were at first not recognized. Pectorals, ventrals and anals were bright salmon. Along the sides and running forward above the eye was a white stripe similar to that of the white sucker. It was more prominent in the darker colored individuals but in none of them was a red stripe visible beneath it as in the white sucker. In a few individuals infrequent, elongated white spots were seen above the lateral stripe and running lengthwise of the back. With field glasses pearl organs were visible on BREEDING BEHAVIOR OF SUCKERS AND MINNOWS. IQ anals and caudal. With good lighting these could be seen with the naked eye at a distance of ten or twelve feet. Subsequent observation showed that all these fish were males. 4. Breeding Activities. — The fish were quiet most of the time, but now and then one dropped downstream a few feet and then slowly returned to his original position. Two of them were seen to pick stones from the bottom as though feeding. After an hour and a half a female joined the group of males. She was longer and relatively thicker bodied than the males, not spotted with white on the back and without visible pearl organs. She was at once approached by five males, two of which took position one on either side of her while the other three crowded down from above. One of the upper males was seen to vibrate his tail for a moment but no actual spawning took place at .the time. The fish re- mained grouped for but an instant and then separated. The female went upstream a little way and then dropped back among the males. The group reformed, but immediately broke up again. In these aborted attempts at spawning it was noted that after a single male had placed himself by the side of a female a second male, upon approaching on the same side, turned at once to the unencumbered side. He behaved as though he discriminated between the sexes of the two fish, but by what means this was accomplished I could not tell. The female again went upstream but this time to a greater distance and followed by two males only. When she had come to rest on the bottom one of the males approached and placed himself by her side. After half a second the other male took his place on her opposite side and spawning occurred very much as in the white sucker (Figs. 5, 6). The backs of the two males were strongly arched so that their dorsal fins were carried well above that of the female and fully spread. Their caudal and anal fins were close pressed against those of the female and against each other as in the wThite sucker. Their snouts were turned inward and pressed close against the sides of the head of the female below her eyes (Figs. 5, 6). While the males were in this position with backs bowed and heads straining inward and upward the female was held firmly by the functional pearl organs of their snouts, caudals, anals 20 JACOB REIGHARD. and tail. The spawning vibrations lasted two or three seconds. The fish then separated and the female went up stream. Repeat- edly after this she dropped down among the males, but when approached by them moved away and did not again spawn. The spawning was not accompanied by any change in color (red stripe) such as was noted in the white sucker. Except in the attitude of the pairing males the breeding be- FIG. 6. Dorsal view of a female of Moxostoma aiireolum pairing with two males. Effective pearl organs are shown. Compare Fig. 5. havior of the red-horse does not differ essentially from that of the white sucker. The eggs are presumably buried in the bottom as in the white sucker. Those of a single female are scattered in small lots over a considerable bottom area. Those fertilized by a single male are also widely scattered. C. The Hogsucker (Calostomus nigricans Le Sueur). i. General Activities. — Catostomus (Hypentelium) nigricans is known locally in Michigan as the black sucker or pugamoo. Over a wider territory it is known as hogsucker, hogmolly or stoneroller. When not breeding it may often be seen feeding on the rapids of our brooks, creeks and smaller rivers. In feeding, the fish puts its snout under a stone and roots it up or thrusts it sidewise. It then sucks up the slime between the stones and with it obtains immature insects which form its chief diet (Forbes and Richard- son, 1908). 1 have once seen the fish when thus engaged each BREEDING BEHAVIOR OF SUCKERS AND MINNOWS. 21 accompanied by ten or twelve small shiners (Notropis) and by an occasional Campostoma. These formed a little school at his sides and below him, and seemed to be waiting for fragments from his feeding. 2. Sexual Differences. — In color the sexes appear to be alike at all times. The back and sides are olivaceous, darker above, with, dark irregular cross blotches. The belly is satiny white; the lower fins are dull red. Thus the colors of the fish blend with those of the stony bottom on which it is commonly seen. I have determined the following percentage differences in fin lengths (percentage D - = (M • - F) : F) for males and females of the same size based on a single female and the average of two males: caudal - 4.5, anal 17.4, dorsal - 0.2, pelvic 4.2, pec- toral - 2.5. These differences are probably within the limits of individual variation except in the case of the anal. The anal of the male thus shows the greatest excess in length over the corresponding fin of the female. The upper and lower caudal lobes are of about equal length in both males and females. This is one of the few species in which pearl organs are de- veloped in both sexes. In the male (Fig. 7, lower male) they occur on both surfaces of all the fins, on the upper surface of the head, on the opercle and on every scale of the body and tail except those of the ventral surface. The largest organs are those on the anal fin, on the ventral half of the caudal and on the sides of the tail, especially near the caudal fin. Those of the anal reach a diameter of 0.8 mm. and a length of 0.28 mm. in a fish 12 cm. in length, while on the caudal of the same speci- men the organs are about 0.5 mm. wide, high and sharp pointed. On the remainder of the fish the organs are small, 0.08 mm. to 0.25 mm. in diameter. All these organs make the surface rough to the touch and are effective in proportion to their size. It is again noteworthy that in the male the anal bears the largest pearls for it is of all the fins the one that shows the greatest percentage of excess length over- that of the female. In the female the organs are smaller than those of the male but have a similar distribution. They are absent from the dorsal fin and sides of the body and from the ventral surfaces of pectoral and pelvic. On the anal they are nearly as large as in the male, while on the 22 JACOB REIGHARD. caudal they are somewhat smaller, still smaller on the sides of the tail. Those on the anal are distinctly perceptible to touch, those on the caudal and sides of the tail are barely perceptible while the rest are quite imperceptible. Probably only those on the anal are in any degree effective and they are not sharp. It is evident that neither the coloration, the length of the fins, nor the pearl organs afford means of discriminating the sexes in the field. For this purpose one is compelled to rely on the difference in average size and in behavior. 3. Breeding Activities.- — I have several times seen a single large hogsucker moving upstream in rapids and accompanied or fol- lowed by three or four smaller. In one case the large fish was some twelve inches long and the four following her half as long. Occasionally she stopped and one of the smaller fish placed himself by her side. But nothing further occurred and the fish presently moved on. My only opportunity to observe the actual spawning of this species was on May 4, 1904, in Mill Creek, near Ann Arbor, at the point at which the spawning of the red-horse had been already seen. I was watching the rapids about 4:30 P.M. when a large hogsucker came upstream followed at a short distance by half a dozen others of two thirds her length. Size and behavior indi- cated the larger fish in this and other cases to be a female. Presently she stopped and remained quiet on the bottom while the males pressed against her three on either side, so close as to hide every part of her except the head and tip of the caudal fin (Fig. 7). The seven fish remained together for several seconds and during this time the female several times made rapidly re- peated movements of protrusion and retraction of the mouth. She was not seen to make any other movement nor was any seen in the males. After remaining thus grouped for a fraction of a minute the fish moved on, the female leading. Two some- what larger males now approached and when the female again stopped these added themselves to the other six, so that the eight of them formed a complete mantle over her back and sides from which only her head and caudal projected. Again the female was seen several times to make rapidly repeated move- ments of the mouth. The fish then passed out of sight on their way upstream, the female still leading. BREEDING BEHAVIOR OF SUCKERS AND MINNOWS. A 2T, 24 17 14 1 1 14 2 O 10 6 I 6 2 I 6lO 6l2 53 64 30 54 21 1 8 14 19 16 9 10 16 22 18 7 18 0 0 I 0 3 5 6 7 4 6 4 3 0 2 0 0 Total .... 135 182 317 58 61 119 43 39 82 40 50 90 3 o 3 19 18 37 9 15 24 ^ 5 ROUGHOID. 37 May 24, 1919. ru + D H + se + + X ru se + + ru se + + 0. i. 2. 3- I, 2. i. 3- 2, 3. I, 2, 3. Culture No. ru D se. H. ru D se. H. se ru. D H. ru se D. H. ru se +. DH. ru se D. H. ru se H. D. ru se H. D. AOA 6l 7=; 24 J2 2^ 'O 2O 0 0 O 4. 12 I 4 I ? 136 66 45 29 0 16 5 6 Summing up the data in the previous tables: Culture No. Total Flies. ru se Value. se D Value. D H Value. 404 . "?CH 2O. O 18.4 18.4 600 . . 247 28.1 17.4 20. 6 6lO 241 2O.^5 I S.^ 24.9 612 . 180 24 8 17. Q 2^.8 Total . . 080 2^.6 17. ^ 21.6 The value for dichaete hairless (21.6) is approximately the expected result (24.8). Sepia dichaete crossing over is noticeably high, since the average normal value is 11.7 units. This is possibly due to a complicating factor, linked to the new factor, roughoid, and "stretching out" that part of the third chromo- some. More work will have to be done before this can be certain. It will be noticed that the double cross-over class (i, 2) is lower than the triple cross-over class (i, 2, 3). This is un- expected and remains unexplained at the present time. SUMMARY. (a) A new mutation has occurred in the third chromosome of Drosophila melanogaster to the left of sepia, which for several years has been the leftmost of the known loci. (b) There was a roughoid sepia cross-over value of 24.9, based on a total of 2,748 flies of which 685 were cross-overs. (c) Associated with roughoid, there is possibly a linkage mod- ifier increasing the crossing over between sepia and dichaete, and perhaps between roughoid and sepia as well. Vol. XXXVIII. February, 1920. No. 2. BIOLOGICAL BULLETIN NOTES ON THE BIOLOGY OF SOME COMMON LAMPYRID^:.1 WALTER N. HESS. CONTENTS. PAGE Introduction 39 History of Biological Work on Lampyrids 40 Photinus consanguineus 41 Photinus scintillans 44 Photurus pennsylvanica 49 Pyropyga fenestralis 67 Purpose of Luminosity 72 Economic Importance , 73 Summary 73 Bibliography 75 INTRODUCTION. The fireflies (family Lampyridse) are among the most common of insects, yet because of the larval habits of most species, com- paratively little is known regarding them except what has been learned from a study of the adults. The larval forms are rarely seen, as most of them are active only at night, and usually are found on, or in the ground, in damp or marshy regions. The insects were observed both in the field and in specially prepared large plant-pots at the insectary. These pots were filled about half full of rich mellow earth on which was placed a small amount of moss. Some of the pots were covered with glass, while others were covered with cheese cloth. Since ovi- position occurred very readily in captivity, eggs were obtained for a study of the incubation periods of the different species, by confining ripe females in small jars that had been partially filled with earth. For a study of the feeding habits of the larvee and adults, these insects were confined in glass jars, some of which 1 Contribution from the Entomological Laboratory of Cornell University. 39 4O WALTER N. HESS. contained only a small amount of moist filter paper, while others contained earth and moss. Since no reference was found to a published account of the nature of the mouth-parts of any of our native firefly larvae, these structures were figured in order to better illustrate the method of feeding of these larvae. The following species were studied: Photinus consanguineus Lee., Photinus scintillans Say, Photurus pennsylvanica DeGeer and Pyropyga fenestralis Mels. The author is indebted to Dr. James G. Needham, Dr. William A. Riley and Dr. O. A. Johannson, under whose supervision the greater part of this study was made, for their helpful suggestions and criticisms. HISTORY OF BIOLOGICAL WORK ON LAMPYRIDS. Although many workers, both in this country and in Europe, have studied the light-organs of the fireflies, comparatively little has been done on the biology of this group of insects. Newport (1857) studied the life-history of the glow-worm, or larva, of Lampyris noctiluca. He not only discussed the de- velopment and general habits of these larvse, but he also per- formed several experiments to determine the nature of their feeding habits. Hudson (1891) published an interesting account of the habits and life-history of the New Zealand glow-worm. Barber (1905, 1914) described the egg-laying habits of Phen- godes, together with certain habits of this group of fireflies. Bongardt (1904) published a brief account of the biology of certain European Lampyridse. Knob (1905) described the habitat, flight and light-emission of Photinus scintillans and Photurus pennsylvanica. McDermott (1910, 1911, 1912, 1914) studied especially the nature of the flashing of fireflies, together with the attraction of the sexes by means of light-emissions. Olivier (1911) described the distinguishing structural char- acters of the common species of fireflies. In addition, he dis- cussed their general habitats and distribution. Mast (1912) studied the sexual attraction of fireflies (Photinus pyralis ?) with special reference to their orientation. He found BIOLOGY OF SOME COMMON LAMPYRID.E. 41 that the female, in responding to the flash of the opposite sex, always turned the ventral side of her abdomen, so that it would emit light in the direction of the male. Fabre (1913) maintained that the glow-worm, in feeding on snails, injected a substance in the nature of an anaesthetic which paralyzed its host. Vogel (1915) did by far the best biological work that has been done on this group of insects. He described the external and internal anatomy of the larva of Lampyris noctiluca, together with its life-history. He observed that the larva lived in the ground, and that it fed on snails. By making a careful study of the structure of the mouth-parts, pharynx and gizzard he found that the digestive juices of the mid-intestine were emitted through the hollow mandibles. By this means the larva was able to paralyze its prey, and to digest the tissues before eating them. Haddon (1915) described also the process of feeding and the nature of the mouth-parts of Lampyris nocliluca. Blair (1915) and Morse (1916) reported the interesting phe- nomenon of the synchronous flashing of fireflies, in which the fireflies in a given locality were found, at times, to flash in unison. Williams (1917) described the life-history of several of our common Lampyrids. His discussions of the biology of Phoiinus consanguineus and Photurus pennsylvanica are especially valuable. Photinus consanguineus Lee. The insects of this species are elongate and slender with the head covered by the prothorax. The prothorax is rounded on the anterior and lateral sides, truncate behind with the angles acute. It is light yellow with a black median bar, which is bordered with pink on either side. The elytra have wide side margins and bear two or three sub-obsolete carinae. The suture and side margins are pale yellow, while the remainder of the elytra is grayish in color. They are granulate and rather pilose. The abdomen in the male is depressed, but in the female it is often rounded, due to being distended with eggs. The eyes of the male are larger and better developed than those of the female. The light-organs of the male cover the entire sternites of the sixth and seventh abdominal segments, while in the 42 WALTER N. HESS. female the organ occupies only a small area on the sixth abdominal segment. They measure from 8 to 12.5 mm. LeConte reports them from Massachusetts, Pennsylvania and Virginia. Blatchley lists them for Indiana, and Williams found them abundant in Massachusetts. The biology of this species has been well described by Williams (1917), so an attempt will be made here to discuss only a few of the more important features. The adults begin to emerge about June I and can be found along moist areas until about the first of August. They are frequently found in association with Photinus scintillans and Photurus pennsylvanica. The males are active fliers, and though the females have well developed wings they were never found in flight. The flight of the males begins about 8:15 P.M. and continues until about 10:00 P.M. (old time). The light of the male is a single bright flash, though at times he emits two or three flashes in rather close succession, but in every case there is a considerable interval between each flash. The female of this species crawls up a stem of grass, or some similar object, and emits a faint flash in response to the flash of the male. Just before emitting the light, however, she turns her abdomen so that the ventral side is in the direction of the male, thus in part, at least, obviating the necessity for larger light-organs. What was discribed as the synchronous flashing of fireflies was first discussed by Blair (1915), who reported observing fireflies, in a certain locality, flashing in unison. Later his observations were confirmed by other writers. Morse (1916) reported an observation in which the light emitted by these little creatures pulsated in a regular synchronous rhythm, so that at one moment the tree, about which they were flying, would be one blaze of light, while at another the light was dim and uncertain. According to Blair (1915) and McDermott (1916), this phe- nomenon does not occur among the American species of Photinus and Photurus. The writer, however, observed the flashing of fireflies in unison on two very dark evenings during the present summer while collecting eggs and larvae at Ithaca, New York. Toward the south side of the City Cemetery is a small valley, BIOLOGY OF SOME COMMON LAMPYRID/E. 43 and on both occasions the entire valley, for a moment, was a blaze of flashing lights, and then for a moment it was in darkness, except for an occasional flash, which seemed to come from fire- flies of different species than those that were flashing in unison. The fireflies in this particular locality were almost entirely of the species Photinus consanguineus , and at each period of flashing both males and females were observed to emit light. On both nights this phenomenon occurred shortly after it became dark, at approximately 9:00 P.M. On the first night the phenomenon was observed for approximately fifteen minutes. How much longer it continued after that was not determined. By very careful observation it was discovered that each period of flashing started on the crest of the hill at the south side of the valley, by one, or only a very few flashes, and that the impulse stimu- lated by these few insects instantly appeared to sweep over the valley, resulting in the great mass of flashing lights. On the second night an experiment was performed in which it was discovered that, by standing on the side of the valley and causing short flashes with a pocket flash-light, the fireflies of the entire valley responded. At first, after estimating the length of the latent period, the flash-light was flashed just before the normal time for the fireflies to flash, with the result that the entire mass of fireflies responded. Then two flashes were emitted from the pocket flash-light with the interval between flashes reduced to about three fourths that of the normal flashing period, and the fireflies responded with apparent equal results. Finally, the period was reduced to approximately one half the normal time. The fireflies as a mass appeared to respond to the first short flash. The second time a large per cent of them responded, »• but after this second short period, the flashing in unison was so disturbed that each insect flashed independently of the flashing period of the others. Blair (1915), in commenting on the reason for the synchronous flashing of fireflies, states: 'The flashing in unison is too regular to be caused by chance puffs of wind. A more probable explanation of the phenomenon is that each flash exhausts the battery, as it were, and a period of recuperation is required before another flash can be emitted. It is then conceivable that the flash of a leader might act as a 44 WALTER N. HESS. stimulus to the discharge of their flashes by the other members of the group, and so bring about the flashing of the whole family." From the observations and experiments performed above, it seems evident that the theory of a leader is the most probable. Although the author has done extensive collecting of these insects during the past four summers, at no time was this phe- nomenon observed on the part of any of our other native species. The small, smooth, spherical eggs are laid on the soil at the base of the roots of grass and moss where they hatch in about twenty to twenty-two days. The larvae are slender, elongate, and of a rather uniform dark grayish color. They were found chiefly a short distance below the surface of the soil, though a few specimens were taken at the surface. It seems, however, that its habitat is subterranean, rather than terrestrial, in con- trast to that of Photurus pennsylvanica. They are predacious, feeding on snails, etc., similar to the other species studied. It seems very evident that the insects have a two-year life cycle, as both mature and half-grown larvae were taken at the time of pupation. Pupation takes place in the soil near the surface of the ground. Here the mature larvae excavate a little chamber, in which the period of transformation is spent. After transforming to pupae they lie on their backs in an arcuate position. In this condition they measure about seven mm., but when straightened out they are about nine mm. long. The pupae are yellowish white in color with the pleural regions somewhat pinkish. The pupal period lasts from twelve to fifteen days. Photinus scintillans Say. The adult beetle of this species is rather elongate and slender'^ somewhat flattened with the head completely covered by the prothorax. The antennae are eleven-segmented, the second seg- ment being short and transverse. The prothorax is rounded an- teriorly and along the sides, truncate behind with the angles acute. The elytra have wide side margins. The head is black. The prothorax is pale yellow, except the small black median bar on its central posterior half, which is bordered with pink. The elytra are pale yellow, except the side and suture margins which BIOLOGY OF SOME COMMON LAMPYRID^. 45 are a lighter yellow. They are finely granulate and pubescent. The females resemble the males except that they are slightly larger and their abdomens, frequently distended with eggs, project considerably beyond the elytra. The eyes of the male are larger than those of the female. In the male the light-organs cover the entire sternites of the sixth and seventh abdominal FIG. i. FIG. 2. FIG. i. Photinus scintillans male, ventral view of abdomen. The shaded portion on the sixth and seventh abdominal segments represents the adult light- organ (AO). FIG. 2. Photinus scintillans female, ventral view of abdomen. The shaded area on the sixth abdominal segment represents the adult light-organ (AO). segments, while in the female the organs occupy only a small area at the center of the sixth sternite (Fig. i and Fig. 2). The beetle measures from 5.5 to 8 mm. These insects were found by LeConte in Massachusetts, Pennsylvania and Kansas. Blatchley found them in Indiana. The natural habitat of this species appears to be very similar to that of Photurus pennsylvanica. Yet, however, they are much more widely distributed, and are often found in drier localities than is characteristic of this species. They are very common about central New York, being our most common firefly. They were found very abundant at Ithaca, New York in the City Cemetery on Stewart Avenue, and, in fact, on the campus, and on the lawns near many of the private houses. The period of emergence for this species varies from about June i to July 20, depending on the season. 46 WALTER N. HESS. During the daytime the adults are rarely found, as they remain hidden underneath the leaves of low herbage, or near the ground covered by grass or moss. Neither sex was observed feeding, so it is not known whether these insects eat as adults or not, but it is probable that they do since they live for two or three weeks. The flight of these insects begins very early in the evening, considerably earlier than that of the other native luminous species. Almost with the first sign of twilight they are flying about, and because it is comparatively daylight the beetles themselves can be distinctly seen. In fact, when they begin flying, their flashes appear rather faint, due to the daylight ob- scuring their brilliancy. Before total darkness sets in, the flight of these insects has ceased, unless for an occasional one here and there. These few stragglers, however, soon stop. Among the fireflies there seems to be, to a certain extent, an evening periodosity of flight. This species of fireflies flies only during twilight, after which other species as Photinus consan- guineus and Photurus pennsylvanica take its place. The flight begins shortly after 7:30 P.M. and continues until about 8:30 P.M. In comparison with most of our luminous species of fireflies, the males of this species fly very low and slowly, and emit one distinct flash at each period of flashing. Females were never taken in flight and it seems probable that they never fly. In- stead, they climb to near the top of some projecting blade of grass, or some similar object, and apparently remain quiet until a male comes near. Like the male, the female emits only one flash, but it is much less distinct. There can be little doubt but that the light-emissions among these insects serve as definite signals between the sexes, by which means the male is able to find the female of his species. Numerous observations confirming this were made, which can be well illustrated by the following example : on the evening of June 30, 1916, a male Photinus scintillans was observed flying about two feet above the ground, and a second after he flashed a female that was almost underneath him flashed. He appeared to drop directly to the ground, but his velocity of flight carried BIOLOGY OF SOME COMMON LAMPYRID^. 47 him about a foot beyond where she was resting. He at once quickly ascended the nearest stalk of grass, seemingly expecting to find her near its top. He then began to descend, at which time he emitted a flash of light and she responded. He then rushed up another stalk of grass that was in her direction and, not finding her, he flashed again. She responded, and he flew in her direction but alighted nearly a foot beyond her, and he then immediately ascended the nearest stalk of grass. Flashes were exchanged for a period of twelve minutes, during which time he ascended about twenty stalks of grass, and he flew in her direction five different times before he finally found her. Each time she responded to his flash, and each time she orientated her abdomen so that the light was emitted in his direction. It was 8:13 P.M. when he found her, and at once copula- tion took place. This lasted until 8:58 P.M. when they separ- ated, and at once crawled down the blade of grass to the ground where they were concealed by the vegetation. On another occasion, while the male was in the grass about a foot from a female in his search to find her, a small pocket flash-light was used, in which it was found that she would readily respond to a very short flash. As soon as the pair was in copulation the flash-light was again flashed several times, at different intervals, but with no response. After the pair separated the flash-light was again flashed, but with no results. After copulation took place neither one of the pair was observed to flash, and after separating they concealed themselves in the grass, without emitting light, all of which seems still further to prove that the light-organs serve to bring the sexes together, and having accom- plished this end, they are no longer functional until at some possible later date. In a very few instances flashing was ob- served on the part of one of the copulating members, but it seemed to occur only when they were disturbed, which was the exception rather than the rule. The pocket flash-light referred to above was used with good success in collecting females. They usually responded when the light was at a distance of at least eight feet, but rarely responded when it was nearer. Oviposition usually took place about one week after emergence 48 WALTER N. HESS. and continued for a period of two or three weeks. As the eggs of the females do not all ripen together, they are deposited over a considerable period of time. Like the other species studied the small, wrhitish eggs were deposited on the ground, at, or near, the base of moss and grass. They were usually laid singly, though in a few instances they were found in masses. When deposited they were covered with an adhesive substance, which caused them to adhere to the object on which they were placed. The period of incubation occupied from eighteen to twenty-one days. In this species, as in all the others- studied, the eggs appeared very slightly luminous at the time of laying, but this faint lumin- osity disappeared in about a day and there was no more light emitted from the eggs until the larval light-organs became func- tional shortly before hatching. The newly hatched larva is whitish, except for the black lateral eyes and brownish mouthparts. It soon becomes pigmented, appearing dull gray in color. At this stage it measures about 2.4 mm. The mature larva resembles in general shape that of the first instar. It is elongate and narrow, varying from 12 to 13 mm. in length. Its head is small, being about half as wide as the prothorax. Like the other species studied, the head can be withdrawn into the thorax. The body is widest in the region of the thorax and tapers gradually posteriorly. The head is black, the tergites dull gray, and the pleural regions slightly pinkish. The habitat of the larva is largely subterranean, though it is usually found near the surface. Its feeding habits resemble very closely those of Photurus pennsylvanica. It is not active during the day. The larvae were not reared from eggs to adults, yet while collecting these insects each spring, larvae of two sizes were found : some that were mature and others that were about half as large, indicating that the insect, probably, has a two-year life cycle. Pupation usually takes place near the surface, although it sometimes occurs under stones. The pupal period is rather brief, taking from nine to twelve days. The pupa assumes an arcuate position, lying on its back within the pupal cell. It BIOLOGY OF SOME COMMON LAMPYRID.E. 49 j measures about 8 mm. in this arcuate position, but when straight- ened out it is about 10 mm. long. The lateral tergites project slightly and each bears a group of short setae. The body is somewhat flattened, being yellowish white in color except along the lateral sides of the thorax and adbomen, which are slightly pink. The larval light-organs function throughout the pupal period and do not degenerate until shortly after the emergence of the adult. The adult light-organs, which develope indepen- dently of the larval organs, become functional shortly before the adults emerge. Plwturus pennsylvanica DeGeer. This is one of the largest of our native fireflies throughout central New York, and though very common in certain moist localities, it is by no means our most common species. The adult insect is elongate, somewhat flattened, with the head partially covered by the thorax. The head is rather rounded, and slightly narrowed behind the large .convex eyes. The antennae are eleven-segmented, slender and tapering, ex- tending about half the length of the body. The prothorax is rounded anteriorly and along the sides, and subtruncate pos- teriorly. It is dull yellow with a central and basal dark stripe, while the disc at each side of the dark area is red. The surface is rather coarsely punctate. The elytra are elongate, extending considerably beyond the end of the abdomen. They are brownish except the lateral margins and a narrow tapering area extending from the anterior part to beyond the center, which is a dull yellow. The body is covered with short yellowish pile. The length ranges from 12 to 15 mm. An illustration of the male is shown on Fig. 3. The sexes are similar in form except that the female is slightly larger than the male. The light-organs of the male cover the entire sternites of the sixth and seventh abdominal segments (Fig. 4), while these organs occupy only about two-thirds of the corresponding region in the female (Fig. 5). In the male the abdomen ends in a point while that of the female is truncate on the tip. This insect is widely distributed throughout North America. 5O WALTER N. HESS. According to LeConte (1851) it is abundant in every part of the United States. Blatchley states that it is the most common firefly in Indiana. Williams also makes a similar statement for Massachusetts. As is characteristic of our luminous fireflies, the adults of this FIG. 3. Phoiurus pennsylvanica male. insect are usually found active only at night. Like many other insects, this species has well-defined centers of distribution, being rarely found except along marshy or moist localities. This, however, is more characteristic of the larvae, but as a rule the adults are found comparatively rarely outside of such regions. They were found most abundant in the City Cemetery, on Stewart Avenue, and in the Renwick marshes, at Ithaca, New York. The cemetery, while not in any sense marshy, has been filled in BIOLOGY OF SOME COMMON LAMPYRID^. with dark loamy soil, which is usually moist in the depressions along the walks and between the graves. The period of emergence for this species, in this locality, extends from June 5 to July 15, depending on weather conditions. During the daytime the adults remain in seclusion, usually at the base of moss or grass, although occasionally specimens were found clinging to the underside of leaves of low vegetation. Many insects during their adult life eat little or no food, but the adults of this species, especially the females, are very vora- FIG. 4. FIG. 5. FIG. 4. Photurus pennsylvanica male, ventral view of abdomen. The shaded portion on the sixth and seventh abdominal segments represents the adult light- organ (AO). FIG. 5. Photurus pennsylvanica female, ventral view of abdomen. The shaded portion on the sixth and seventh abdominal segments represents the adult light- organ (AO). cious in their feeding habits. During the evening, while they are active, they are either found flying, or on the ground, usually about the base of grass, actively in search of food. The females were commonly observed devouring other species of fireflies (Photinus scintillans, Photinus marginellus and Photinus con- 52 WALTER N. HESS. sanguineus). In each case, however, the firefly being devoured was a male, which had probably attracted the Photurus by means of its frequent flashing, as the females of these species flash very rarely, except in response to the flash of the male. In captivity the female was often found devouring the males of her own species, and occasionally a member of her own sex. The males were not found feeding, though it seems probable that they also are predatory at times. Though both sexes fly readily, comparatively few females were found on the wing during the early part of the season, although later (about July first to fifteenth), when it seemed evident that there was a less abundance of males, the females were found flying fully as much as those of the opposite sex. The flight began about 8:15 P.M. and continued until after 10:00 P.M., although by that time comparatively few were flying. Some have been observed on the wing as late as I :oo A.M. Unlike our other native species, these fireflies frequently fly high and their flashes can often be seen in the tops of the highest trees. The flashing of this firefly is very distinctly different from that of any of our other native species. Like the other fireflies, these have definite periods of flashing. The male flashes three, four, and even five times in rapid succession at each period. The flashes are bright, although they become less distinct at the end of each period. In the case of the female the number of flashes at each period is reduced to three, two or one. There is usually a longer period between the flashes than in the male and they are less distinct. These periods of flashing occur at rather regular intervals of about eight to ten seconds. The male flashes slightly more frequently than the female. When on the ground the brilliancy and regularity of flashing, on the part of both sexes, seems to be the same as when they are flying. It is agreed by most students of fireflies that the light-emissions serve to bring the two sexes together, although McDermott (1911) and Williams (1917) seem to doubt that they can serve such a function in this species. In the other native luminous species that were studied there is a definite interchange of flashes, in which the female responds to the flash of the male. In Photurus the female is an active flier and flashes frequently, BIOLOGY OF SOME COMMON LAMPYRID^i. 53 whether in the presence of the male or not. In no case was there observed a definite exchange of flashes between the sexes, yet it seems very evident that the light-emission functions in bringing the sexes together. On several occasions, while holding females in my hand, males flew to them and they would have alighted had my presence not scared them away, and on two occasions, while holding males, females flew and alighted beside the cap- tured males. This would lead one to believe that there is a definite sexual attraction by means of light-emissions between the sexes of this species, and that the female, having become an active flier, is also attracted to the male. In each case observed where the two sexes were attracted to one another, both con- tinued to flash actively, but in no case was there any evidence that one was responding to the flash of the other. In no case did the female of this species assume a vertical position or expose the abdomen so that the light would be flashed in the direction of the male. It is possible that the brilliancy of the light-emissions on the part of the female has obviated the necessity for such exposure. Even while the females were on the ground the flashes were easily perceptible for a considerable distance. Copulation was observed in the field on three occasions. Unlike our other native fireflies, these beetles were never observed in copula on, or near, the ground, but while clinging to the leaves of trees, often at considerable height. While in this state the flashing of the light-organs apparently ceased, though one female was found emitting a rather dim continuous glow during this period. Egg laying usually began about one week after emergence and continued at intervals for a period of about two or three weeks. Several females, which were captured early in the season, were dissected to determine their egg laying capacity. On the average, each female contained about fifty mature eggs, with from seventy-five to a hundred smaller ones, indicating that the eggs were at varying stages of maturity. The characteristic place for the oviposition by the female is at the base of grass or moss in damp loamy soil. Oviposition was not observed in the field, yet on several occasions eggs were found which had been deposited at the base of the roots of grass 54 WALTER N. HESS. and moss. In confinement, at the insectary, oviposition was observed on several occasions. The female walked slowly over the soil, thrusting out her long ovipositor into the depressions in the earth, where the light yellowish eggs were deposited. Eggs laid by these females were usually deposited singly, though sometimes in masses. Some were placed from one eighth to a quarter of an inch underneath the surface, others on the surface, and some were deposited on the roots, basal stems and leaves of moss and grass. The egg (Fig. 6) is FIG. 6. Photurus pennsylva- nicaegg. small, nearly spherical, about .7 by .8 mm. in diameter. It is without surface markings, though at the time of laying it is covered with an adhesive surface. It is frequently stated in literature that the eggs of fireflies are luminous, and Williams (1917) states that the eggs of this species glow when deposited and probably continue to emit light until the time of hatching. The certainty of this statement seems in doubt. At the time of laying, the eggs were found to be very slightly luminescent for a period of two days, but in no case did they, at this period nor until the light-organs of the larva were developed, definitely emit light. Eggs removed from the ovaries of a ripe female showed no evidence of luminosity, though Williams thought he saw a light in some of the eggs that he so removed. Fabre (1913) states that the eggs are lumin- ous even before leaving the body of the mother, but this seems very much in doubt. The so-called luminescence of the eggs at the time of laying is probably due to the substance with which they are covered, rather than to any internal property of their own, and as this becomes dry the slight luminescence disappears. At a period about four days before the eggs hatch the larval light-organs become functional, and from this time until hatching, the eggs emit a distinct light. In the breeding cages at the insectary where normal outdoor conditions were maintained, the eggs of this species hatched in from twenty-five to twenty-seven days, depending on weather BIOLOGY OF SOME COMMON LAMPYRID.E. 55 conditions at different periods. The largest number hatched on the twenty-sixth day. The newly hatched larvae are not pigmented for a period of a few hours, but they soon resemble the mature larva in shape and appearance, except for their mina- ture size. At this stage they meas- ure about 2.25 mm. in length. As nearly as could be determined the larvae were in the fourth instar by winter. The first two molts take place rather early in the life of the larva, the first occurring at about the age of two or three weeks. The mature larva (Fig. 7) is elon- gate, rather narrow, varying from 16—19 mm. in length, and it is about three times as long as it is wide. The head is small, a little less than one third as wide as the prothorax, and it can be withdrawn into a pouch within the thorax. The Antennae are three-jointed, the mandibles are arcuate and notched near the middle. The legs are some- what spinose and of nearly uniform length. The body is much flat- tened. The prothorax is rounded on the anterior and lateral sides and subtruncate behind, much the same as in the adult. This is the largest of the body segments. From the metathoraeic segment to the caudal end of the larva the ter- gites are concave posteriorly espec- ially the caudal ones. Each bears a spine on its caudo-lateral margin. The head, mouth-parts and tergites are colored a dark brown, except for a few irregular pale yellowish areas. The dorsal surface is more or less irregularly coarsely punctate. The last segment of the abdomen is provided with numerous retrac- FIG. 7. Photunis pennsylvanica larva, full grown, dorsal view. WALTER N. HESS. LO— tile elements, the caudal filaments (Fig. 8, C, F), which are used in propelling the body forward. On the lateral sides of the eighth abdominal sternite are two luminous areas, the larval light-organs (LO}. Resembling to a certain extent the habits of the adults, these larvae unless disturbed were not found active during the daytime. At this time they are usually under- neath stones or concealed in de- pressions in the ground. At night they become active and their light can be frequently seen as they wander about in their natural hab- itat. When the ground is smooth a small garden rake is of advantage in collecting them, as they can be easily disturbed by it, thus expos- ing their light-organs to view. The glow of their light-organs is not visible when the larva? are lying on the ground, as it can only be seen when the ventral sides of their FIG. 8. Photurus pennsylvanica larva, ventral view of abdomen, abdomens are exposed to view. { A LO, larval light-organ; CF, caudal flash-light is also of assistance in filaments' collecting them. While in the field, collecting on numerous occasions during the evening, larvae were found wandering about apparently in search of food. On two occasions they were taken while feeding on snails, which they had evidently killed a short time before being discovered. As the larvae crawled about, their heads were fully distended, the maxillary palpi and antennas were constantly in motion, and it appeared as if they were feeling their way by means of these organs. From observations on their movements, even when a snail was very near, it seemed very probable that the larvae find their food by chance, and having found it they tap it several times with their maxillary palpi and antennae before beginning to feed. Since newly hatched larvae and those one- year-old were abundant, efforts were made to determine more in detail the methods of feeding and the nature of their food. .--C F BIOLOGY OF SOME COMMON LAMPYRIDJE. 57 Larvae taken into the laboratory were placed under as normal conditions as possible, where various experiments were per- formed to determine the possible nature of their food. In no case were newly hatched larvae found in the act of killing their prey, though they were observed feeding on bits of snail that was cut up and placed near them. They fed much the same as the older larvae, and, in fact, there can be little doubt but that their food habits are similar. As the larvae were not active during the daytime, and as they were disturbed by artificial light, my observations on the feeding habits were largely limited to the larvae of Pryopyga fenestralis which is active during the day. On six different occasions a slug (Agriolimax campestris Binney) was placed with six larvae of Photurus pennsylvanica and in every case it had been eaten before morning. A slug (Agriolimax agrestris L.) and a snail (Succinea avery Say) were put in with six larvae. The snail was eaten during the first night, but the slug was not killed and eaten until the third night. On two occasions a small earthworm (Lumbricus terrestris L.) was placed in a jar, without earth, which contained eight larvae. One was killed and eaten the second night, and the other on the fifth night. On two occasions a very large specimen of Limbricus lerrestris was placed with twelve larvae. In each case the earth- worm was not disturbed, though it remained with the firefly larvae for over a week, and they received no other food during that time. On two occasions a potato-beetle larva (Leptinotarsa decemlineata Say) was placed in a jar with six larvae and each time it was eaten the first night. On two occasions cutworm larvae (Paragrotis messoria Harris, Paragrotis tessellata Harris and Peridroma margaritosa Haworth) were each placed in jars with six larvae and in every instance they were eaten the first night. Finally, on four different nights, two second and two third stage squash-bug nymphs (Anasa tristis DeGeer) were placed with six larvae, and in each instance they were eaten before morning. Sowbugs (Oniscus asellus Paulmeier), wire- worm larvae (Agriotes mancus Say), ants (Formica sp.) and coleop- terous beetles including the common ground beetles (Nebia pallipes Say and Chcelnius pennsylvanicus Say) were placed in with these larvae, but they were never eaten, indicating that the 58 WALTER N. HESS. larvae require a soft-bodied animal into which they can pierce their mandibles and inject the poisonous secretion. These results, while giving no definite data as to the exact food of these larvae, lead one to conclude that they probably eat any soft-bodied insect larva, mollusca or annelid, that they happen to find in their nocturnal wanderings. Snails are probably one of their chief foods, and though these animals are not supposed to be very abundant, they were found abundantly at night in the damp regions where these larvae live. Cutworm larvae were also abundant. Earthworms, except very small ones, were not eaten until they had been with the larvae for a considerable length of time, while other food was eaten very readily. This would seem to indicate that probably snails and small insect larvae, expe- cially cutworms, are their natural foods. Among the larvae of many members of the Lampyridas, as well as among certain other more or less widely separated groups of insects, digestion takes place entirely, or partially, outside of the body. This is accomplished by the digestive juices being exuded from the mouth upon the food which is later eaten by the larvae in a more or less completely digested condition. 'It is characteristic of most insects that feed in this way to have FIG. 9. Photurus pennsylvanica larva, labrum, dorsal view. very small heads, so that the mouth is not sufficiently large to take in only very small pieces of food. These insects, like the larvae of the Dytiscida?, are predaceous, feeding on living animal food. The nature of this food is such that it could not be easily BIOLOGY OF SOME COMMON LAMPYRID.E. 59 AO chewed, like vegetable food or decayed animal tissues, and hence it seems that some such method of feeding became necessary. Some have the mandibles grooved, as the larvae of the Chry- sopidse and Myrmeleonidae, while others have them pierced by a small canal, such as certain of the larvae of the Dytiscidse and Lampyridae. The most extensive work on this subject was done by two Euro- pean workers, Vogel (1912, 1915) and Haddon (1915), although earlier work- ers made a less detailed study of the problem. When the head of this insect is withdrawn into the thorax, only the tips of the mandibles and other mouth- parts are visible, but when it is ex- tended the large mandibles surrounded by the other mouth-parts can be dis- tinctly seen. As the anterior half of the mandib- les are exposed on the dorsal side, the labrum lies considerably caudad on the head, extending across the basal FIG. 10. Plioturus pennsyl- portions of these large jaws. It is mw/ca larva, left mandible, dor- rounded On the lateral margins, while sal view. AO, anterior opening on its cephalic border are three prom- of mandibuiar canal; c, con- r . . ... dyle; PO, posterior opening of inent forward projecting portions with .... . ~ . ,, mandibuiar canal; T, tooth on rather acute terminations. Its dorsal inner edge of mandible. side (Fig. 9), shows numerous long projecting bristles while the ventral side is covered with a rather dense mass of small setae which project forward. The mandibles (Fig. 10) are very strong. Each has a large curved, anterior tapering tooth and they meet in a median line slightly anterior of the head. On the inner median margin of each mandible is a secondary tooth (7"). At the base is a knob- like condyle (C) by which the mandible articulates with the head. Both mandibles are covered for their entire extent with setae of varying length, except at the distal end, and around the condyle. About the base is a dense brush of short setae which project for- po 6o WALTER N. HESS. ward. Between this area and the secondary tooth there are larger and stiffer setae. Those covering the remainder of the surface are rather short and dense. Extending from the base to near its tip is a tubular canal, through which digestive juices FIG. ii. Photurus pennsylvanica larva, labium, maxillae and lacinia, ventral view. LP, labial palpus of labium; M, mentum of labium; SM, submentum of Jabiuni; MP, maxillary palpus; ST, stipes of maxilla; Cd, cardo of maxilla; L lacnia; G, galea. pass, while the larva is feeding. It does not open at the tip but slightly caudad on the outer margin. The maxillae, labium and lacinia lie on the ventral side of the head, and in these larvae they are fused into a flat fleshy plate (Fig. n). When examined on its ventral side, the maxilla has at its caudal portion a small triangular plate, the cardo (Cd), which bears several short setae. Anterior to it is the large elon- BIOLOGY OF SOME COMMON LAMPYRID^. 6l gated stipes (St), which also bears several long bristles and many shorter setae. Anterior of the stipes on the external side is a stout four-segmented palpus, the maxillary palpus (MP), bearing several forward projecting setae. At the side of the maxillary palpus, internally, is the two-segmented galea (G) of the maxilla, which resembles very much in appearance a two- segmented palpus. Beside the galea, internally and also extend- ing a short distance along the stipes, is the rather dense chitinized flattened lacinia (L). Both the galea and lacinia bear many setae, and along the inner margin of the lacinia is a row of rather stiff bristles. The tip of the galea ends in short setae, which probably function as specialized sense organs. On the ventral side of the labium, the submentum (SM) appears much elongated and it lies in the central region of the mouth-parts. It bears numerous short setae and a few long bristles. The mentum (M) is in the form of a thickened bi-lobed struc- ture, from which project anteriorly the two-segmented labial palpi (LP). Both structures bear a considerable number of anteriorly projecting setae and bris- tles. On the underside, the sclerites of FIG. 12. Photums pennsyi- the maxillae, labium and lacinia are in- vanica larva- hypopharynx. ... ... . dorsal view. distinct, though in this region there are numerous small anteriorly projecting setae. The hypopharynx (Fig. 12) is triangular in shape with the anterior portion bi-lobed. It is entirely covered with a dense mass of anteriorly projecting setae, except along its basal portion. The setae of the anterior half are much denser and longer, and many of them are branched. From the previous description of the mouth-parts, it is evident that there are numerous setae projecting forward inside of the mouth region. These setae, according to Haddon (1915) and Vogel (1915), function as a strainer in preventing all except liquid food from entering the mouth. In the pharynx region there are several heavily chitinized plates to which numerous muscles are attached, and which, 62 WALTER N. HESS. according to these same authors, function as a suction pump in drawing the liquid food into the mouth. Vogel also described how the mid-intestinal secretions are forced forward, by means of heavy oblique and circular pro-ventricular muscles, into the mouth and out through the mandibular canals. Though the intestinal juices were not observed passing through the canals of the mandibles, a rather dark-colored liquid was seen suspended from the ends of the long curved teeth and on the bodies of snails and earthworms in the region where the larvae were feeding. It, therefore, seems very probable that a certain liquid, which, since the larvae have no salivary glands, must come from the region of the mid-intestine, is exuded through the canals of their mandibles and out through the mouth, and that this liquid functions in paralyzing and digesting the tissues of their prey. The larvae were observed, however, to take into their mouths portions of food of considerable size. The fact that they can take up mouthfuls of earth and masticate it in the construction of their pupal-cells is evidence that the larvae are able to take into their mouths small masses of food before it is completely digested. Yet there can be little doubt but that the-greater part of the food is digested outside of the body and taken in through the mouth, in the liquid state. Whether the mandibles function in the intake of food was not determined, but the greater part of it was apparently taken in through the mouth. The portions of undigested food that were taken into the mouth, were no doubt largely digested here before passing en into the intestine, as the larvee masticated these masses for a considerable time before they disappeared. The larval light-organs are fully developed at birth, so the larva is luminous from the time that it hatches until it finally enters the pupal state. As stated before, the light is emitted from two elliptical areas on the ventral side of the eighth ab- dominal segment. The larval light-organs do not emit light in flashes, as the organs of many adults do, but on the other hand, the glow is nearly uniform. While the larvae were active, the light was found to glow continuously. During the dormant periods, as during the day, but especially during hibernation, BIOLOGY OF SOME COMMON LAMPYRID^. 63 the glow becomes very faint and it frequently is not perceptible even while holding the larvse ventral side up in a dark room. By moving the insects about so as to agitate them the lights usually become visible. It seems probable that the brilliancy of the glow is in direct proportion to the activity of the larvse. The larvae which were kept in confinement at the insectary were found to go underneath stones, or enter cracks in the soil, late in October, in preparation for hibernation. Some constructed about themselves earthen chambers, while others occupied natural depressions in these protected places. In no instance were they found lying on their dorsal side, such as is characteristic of the pupae. During the warm nights of April the larvae leave their winter quarters and go about in search of food. At this season their little lights can again be seen as they wander about in their natural habitats at night. Since this species of insect lives as a larva at the base of grass in moist loamy soil, and since it does not enter the ground, or seek other natural means of concealment in which to pass the •• l-v... FIG. 13. Photuriis pennsylvanica partially constructed pupal chamber. pupal period, as many members of the Coleoptera, as well as some Lampyrids do, it constructs a small earthen chamber in which to pass this period of transformation. A suitable spot on the surface of the ground, usually at the base of moss or grass is chosen, and at once the larva begins building a lattice work of soft earth over itself, in the shape of a small dome (Fig. 13), by which means it conceals itself , in about a day. In the construction of this cell the larva removes earth from underneath itself by means of its mandibles. This it masticates and mixes in its mouth for a period of about half a minute. It then extends its head to the lattice work of the 64 WALTER N. HESS. dome and regurgitates the moist earth in the form of a short ribbon-like mass, which it applies to the walls of the chamber. By the frequent repetition of this process, the lattice-like frame- work finally entirely covers the larva (Fig. 14). Even after it forms a complete dome, the larva can be seen for several hours between the meshes, before it is entirely concealed. By re- peatedly removing the earth from the bottom of the chamber FIG. 14. Pholurus pennsylvanica pupal chamber completed, lateral view. and adding it to the inside of the dome-like wall, the chamber is deepened, and its covering is strengthened and made thicker. The completed chamber is in the form of an elliptical depres- sion in the ground, about one half of an inch in length and about seven sixteenths of an inch in width. Even after the larva con- ceals itself it continues to add to the walls of the cell until they are from one eighth of an inch, to as much as one half of an inch in thickness. The time spent in building the pupal-cell is about two days, though larvae sometimes continue to excavate for three or even four days, making a firmer and thicker covering for their cells. That the intestinal secretions of the larva are used for moist- ening the earth, which is used in constructing the cell, there can be little doubt, yet it evidently has no special adhesive content, for the pupal-cells are easily broken, and they seem to offer no more resistance than ordinary earth which has dried, after having been mixed with water. This liquid seems to serve simply as a fluid in which to mix the earth and make it plastic. From the examination of several of these completed pupal- cells, it seems evident that the method of construction is such as to allow a small amount of air for respiration to pass in and out between their meshes. Some of the domes of the completed BIOLOGY OF SOME COMMON LAMPYRID/E. 65 cells, when held against the light, allowed small rays to pass through, giving evidence of their slight porous nature. The pupal-cell, however, makes a sufficiently well-constructed cham- ber to protect the pupa from drying or other injury. The reason that the larva usually seeks a damp locality previous to pupation is probably for the purpose of choosing a place where excessive drought will not be liable to affect it during trans- formation. By the time the pupal-cell is constructed the larva becomes very sluggish, its body becomes distended and in from one to three days the cuticula splits down the anterior half of the back, and the pupa gradually comes forth. From this time, FIG. 15. Photurus pennsylvanica pupal chamber completed, internal view with pupa in position. throughout the entire pupal stage, it lies on its dorsal side within its pupal-cell, and is largely supported by the long lateral setae, which project from each of the thoracic and abdominal tergites (Fig- 15)- The straightened out pupa measures about twelve mm. and in the arcuate position ten mm. The body is somewhat de- pressed, with the appendages and wing-pads rather long, and with the lateral tergites drawn out at considerable length. At the end of each of these appendages is a mass of coarse bristles. The color of the pupa is yellowish white. It is quite active and can move about considerably within its pupal-chamber. Throughout the entire pupal period the light-organs, that were functional in the larva, can be distinctly seen to emit light, 66 WALTER N. HESS. although they do not shine with a bright luminescence unless the pupa moves, or is disturbed (Fig. 16). The head, thorax, and even the abdomen of the pupa, as well as the newly emerged adult, have been described as luminous (Williams, 1916). It must be admitted that the entire insect appears faintly luminous at these periods, but the cause for it LO —- LO FIG. 16. FIG. 17. , FIG. 16. Photurus pennsylvanica pupa, ventral view of abdomen. LO, larval light-organ. FIG. 17. Same as Fig. 2, except taken one day after emergence when larval light-organs (LO) were still visible. AO, adult light-organ. seems to be due more to the light of the abdominal light-organs shining through the non-pigmented coverings of the insect's body, than to the luminosity of the fat or other internal struc- tures. A freshly molted larva appears much the sam«. as the two stages refened to above. The date of pupation for this species, at Ithaca, New York, ranges from May 20 to June 15, depending largely on the season. BIOLOGY OF SOME COMMON LAMPYRID^E. 67 The extent of the pupal period was found to vary from sixteen to eighteen days under outdoor conditions. From the fifth of June to about the first of July the pupae may be found transforming to adults within their pupal-cells. On the first or second night, after transformation, the adult ruptures the pupal-cell and comes forth as a mature insect. The larval light-organs which are functional during the pupal stage continue to glow until the end of the second day of adult life, when they become fainter and fainter and cease to function. One species that was reared did not show evidence of the adult organ until after emergence, and then only in the sixth abdominal segment, although the organ in segment seven began to glow a few hours later. Since the adult light-organs are functional at the time of emergence and since the larval light-organs function for a period of about two days of adult life, there is a brief period during which both organs are luminous (Fig. 17). Pyropyga fenesiralis Mels. This beetle is elongate, oval and slender. It is entirely black or blackish except for the large pinkish subtriangular space on each side of the black central disc of the prothorax. The head is completely covered by the prothorax. The eyes are small in both sexes. The antennae are eleven-segmented and slender. The elytra are costate. The sexes are similar except that the female is considerably larger than the male, and her abdomen which is usually distended extends beyond the elytra. The length varies from 6.5 to 10 mm. LeConte reports these insects from Pennsylvania, Lake Superior Region, Colorado and California. Blatchley found them plentiful in Indiana. The adults of these fireflies differ from those of the species previously referred to in this paper, in that they are active only during the daytime and not at night. This change in habit can probably be accounted for by the absence of light-organs in the adults. With this one exception, their general habits are similar to those of the other species studied. They are never found in dry localities, being chiefly found in low meadows along streams or marshy areas. 68 WALTER N. HESS. The period of emergence is more prolonged than is charac- teristic of most species, varying from June 15 to August 10. During the day these insects can usually be found clinging to grass and weeds by the side of streams. The males are active fliers but the females were not taken in flight, yet it is possible that they fly, for they are sometimes found at a considerable distance from their natural habitat. The adults were never observed feeding. If they feed at all it is probably very sparingly, for specimens were kept in the laboratory without food from the time of emergence through the period of oviposition. Copulation was observed to take place from two to ten days after emergence, with frequent repetitions. In the field the copulating pairs were always found about a foot from the ground on a blade of grass or some similar object. The female, early in the season, is usually distended with eggs in varying stages of maturity. Egg laying starts about four to five days after emergence, and continues for a period of two to three weeks. The females of this species also lay their eggs on the ground at the base of vege- tation in damp ground. In every case observed, the eggs were deposited singly. The egg is small, spherical, whitish in color, measuring about .65 mm. in diameter. It is without surface markings and when first deposited it is covered with an adhesive substance. These eggs, while they are in no sense luminous, give off a slight lumines- cence when first laid.. Under normal conditions they hatch in from nineteen to twenty-one days into little whitish, elongated larvae. The larvae, when first hatched, resemble very closely in appear- ance those of Photinus scintillans, in that they are whitish with dark eyes and brownish mandibles. At this stage they measure about 2.3 mm. in length. On becoming pigmented, however, they appeared dark gray in color. The mature larva is elongate, narrow, varying from 12 to 14 mm. in length. The blackish head is small, being about half as wide as the prothorax. The body is widest in the region of the thorax and tapers posteriorly. The tergites are dull blackish BIOLOGY OF SOME COMMON LAMPYRID.^. 69 in color, while the pleural regions are distinctly pinkish. On the ventro-lateral sternite of the eighth abdominal segment are two luminous areas, the larval light-organs. Similar to the adults of this species the larvae are active during the day, though as a rule they are more active at night, when they can be found wandering about on the ground at the edge of streams apparently in search of food. Frequently, however, while searching for them during the day they were found con- cealed under stones and in the ground to a depth of from one to two inches. On several occasions numbers of them were found assembled together, where they were feeding on a captured snail or earthworm. They were found most abundant at Ithaca, N. Y., on the gravel at the edge of Cascadilla Creek a short dis- tance below the new fish hatchery. Since these larvae were active during the day-time, and as they readily took food, it seemed advisable to study more in detail their food habits. Newport (1857), Meinert (1886), Fabre (1913), Haddon (1915) and Vogel (1915) each studied the food habits of Lampyris nocliluca. Newport evidently did not observe closely the man- dibles, for he did not mention their hollow nature, which was observed by the other four workers. Newport maintained that the bite of the larva definitely injured the snail upon which it was feeding, while Fabre took a different view, maintaining that the larva injected a substance into its host in the nature of an anaesthetic which paralyzed it, thus making it possible for the larva to feed without being disturbed by the efforts of the snail in trying to escape. So far as my observations go, I am inclined to believe that the larva does inject a definite substance which serves to paralyze and finally to kill its host. On one occasion a rather small earthworm, about three inches long, was placed with six larvae. In about two minutes one of the larvae, as it moved about, feeling its way with its maxillary palpi and antennae, came in contact with the worm. It touched it several times with these structures, which undoubtedly were supplied with sense organs, and then pierced the worm with its mandibles. The earthworm quickly moved and was evidently slightly injured, but it soon 7O WALTER N. HESS. became quiet. The larva soon released and bit the worm again in the same region. This time the worm moved much less. The process was repeated several times, and each time the worm was less disturbed, until at the sixth or seventh bite the earth- worm was not aware of the larva's presence. At about this time another firefly larva bit the worm about an inch away from where the first one was feeding. This part of the earthworm was sensitive, and the worm, though much less active, responded much the same as before. About two minutes later a third larva bit the worm in a still different region, with the result that the worm moved a little but much less than for the other two larvae. Ten minutes from the time that the first larva bit the worm it apparently was perfectly paralyzed, and so far as could be determined it was dead. Several slugs and snails were observed in the process of being killed by these larvae. As the reaction of these snails is similar to that of the earthworm, it is sufficient to state that at first the snail appeared to be slightly injured, for in every instance it contracted. In each case, a few minutes after the larva began feeding, the snail was evidently paralyzed for it no longer moved. One of the largest specimens of earthworms obtainable (about seven inches long) was placed in a jar containing moist filter paper with twelve larvae. Although the worm was left with the larvae for six days, and though they were given no other food there was no evidence of any effort being made to eat it. While feeding on its prey, the larva keeps its jaws actively moving back and forth, apparently ejecting digestive juices from its mid-intestine by way of the mandibular canal, and with the two inner teeth on the mandibles it tears the worm to pieces and draws small portions into its mouth. At times the larva would almost bury itself inside the body of its prey, apparently preferring the softer internal tissues to those of the exterior. From an examination of the mouth and intestinal contents of larvae, which were killed while they were feeding, and from careful observation of their feeding habits, it seems evident that a certain amount of digestive juices are exuded through the mandibular canals into the host, when it is first attacked, which serve to paralyze it. It is also probable that a certain amount of the BIOLOGY OF SOME COMMON LAMPYRID^. 71 digestive juice is constantly being exuded through the mouth as well as by these canals, while the larva is feeding, which serves to break down and partially digest the tissues of the host. Fur- ther, it was evident that small portions of undigested flesh, as well as food that was nearly digested, was taken into the mouth, where it was bathed in the digestive juices and worked back and forth by means of the two mandibular teeth, and that liquid food together with very small portions of partially digested food passed into the crop and intestinal region where digestion was completed. As far as could be determined, the mandibular canals did not serve for the intake of foods, as in the Chrysopidae, but they appeared to function, as far as digestion is concerned, simply as canals for exuding digestive fluids into the host. At frequent intervals while feeding, certain of the larvae were observed to extend the caudal filaments, twist the body around, and apply them to the portion of food that was being eaten. It appeared as if the larva was placing some glandular secretion upon the food, or possibly helping to push it into its mouth. Other experiments were performed to determine whether or not these larvae limited their food to snails and earthworms. On successive days cutworm larvae (Peridroma margaritosa Haworth, Paragrotis tessellata Harris), potato-bettle larvae (Leptinotarsa decemlineata Say), squash-bug nymphs (Anasa tristis DeGeer) and wireworms (Agriotes mancus Say) were placed in the pots with these firefly larvae. They readily ate all of the cutworm larvae, but in no instance were any of the other insects eaten. From the above experiments it seems evident that these firefly larvae feed chiefly on snails, cutworms and small earth- worms. The other insects that were offered them as food were chosen because they are sometimes found on, or near the ground, and it was desired to see how wide a range of food they would eat. Although the adults have no light-organs, the larvae have well developed light-organs which resemble very closely, in appearance, those of the other species studied. It, however, appears to be somewhat smaller and emits a less distinct light. Larvae were not reared from the egg to the adult, so the length of their life-cycle cannot be given with certainty. Yet, since half- 72 WALTER N. HESS. grown larvae were found during July and August in association with the mature ones, it seems very probable that the larvae live for two years before transforming to adults. Pupation takes place from about June 10 to August i. The larvae do not build an elaborate pupal-cell, as the larva of Pho- turus pennsylvanica does, but on the other hand, they crawrl back away from the edge of the water, three to ten feet, where they go underneath stones, and there excavate little cells in which the pupal period is spent. The mature larva shortens up slightly and assumes an arcuate position. The cuticula splits down the middle of the dorsal thoracic region and gradually liberates the pupa. The pupa, except for the yellowish white head, appendages and tip of the abdomen, is of a delicate roseate color. The pleural regions, however, are decidedly pinkish. The abdominal and thoracic tergites are drawn out ventro-laterally into rather acute projections. At the end of each is a mass of setae. The straightened out pupa measures 8 mm., and in an arcuate position, 6.5 mm. The pupal period is rather brief, extending for only seven or eight days. The larval light-organs function throughout the pupal period, but as the time for emergence approaches, the glow usually becomes very faint. In a few specimens the light was observed in the adults for a brief period after emergence. PURPOSE OF LUMINOSITY. There can be little doubt but that the chief function of light- emission in insects is to assist in securing the mating of the sexes. It is evident that this has come about as a secondary character when one considers the varying degrees to wrhich the light-organs are developed among Lamyprids. It was suggested by Blair (1915) that possibly the light may be an indication of impalata- bility. In the case of the adults of Photinus scintillans, this does not seem probable, as one frequently finds the females of Photurus pennsylvanica and numerous species of spiders feeding upon them. Many adult fireflies when captured emit a pale yellow fluid from between the last coxal joints and from the BIOLOGY OF SOME COMMON LAMPYRID/E. 73 pygidium, which it seems may be impalatable to many of their enemies. In the case of luminous larvae there seems to be little possible use for light-organs, unless it is to warn any possible enemies not to eat them. There are many species of insects outside of the family Lampyridae that resemble very closely in appearance certain species of fireflies. If the explanation of this mimicry is that of protection, it would seem that the theory of impalatability must have some basis. The exact purpose for the presence of light-organs in larvae is still indefinitely understood. ECONOMIC IMPORTANCE. Practically no work has been done on the possible economic value of these insects, and though the data here given are little more than suggestive, every evidence seems to indicate that these insects are of considerable economic importance. The adults are of little value, as most of them probably eat compara- tively little, but the larvae are voracious little creatures which live on and in the ground, and feed on snails, earthworms, cutworm larvae, and, in fact, on larvae of many injurious insects. Most of the soft-bodied animals living on the ground are injurious (unless it is the earthworms), and as the food of firefly larvae is probably limited to these small animals, they necessarily do much economic good in killing them. The slugs and cutworm larvae are among our worst economic pests, and it seems evident that they furnish a large part of the food of these larvae. Since most of the fireflies live two years as larvae, the number of larvae that are feeding on the ground during any season is approximately twice that of the adult fireflies. Anyone who has been out during a June or July evening knows that the fireflies are one of our most abundant insects, which, together with the voracious habits of these larvae leads to the belief that they are of much more economic importance than has been attributed to them heretofore. SUMMARY. 1. The fireflies studied are luminous both in the larval and adult states, except Pyropyga fenestralis, which is luminous only during larval life. 2. In the luminous species the light-organ of the male is 74 WALTER N. HESS. better developed and more brilliant than that of the female. In Photurus pennsylvanica, however, the light-organ in the female is nearly as large and as brilliant as that of the male, but in the other luminous species the organ of the female is limited to a small area on the sixth abdominal segment and it emits a rather faint light. 3. The light-organs of the adults undoubtedly function chiefly in bringing the sexes together. The nature of the flash differs for each species, so that members of the same species can readily recognize the flash of the opposite sex. The males and females of certain species, at times, respond to the light of a small flash- light, indicating that this is not always true. 4. In all these luminous species, except Photurus pennsyl- vanica, the female orientates her abdomen so that the light emission is in the direction of the male. 5. In all the species studied the larval light-organ degenerates and, in the luminous species, separate light-organs are developed, which function during adult life. 6. In the case of Photinus consanguineus, synchronous flashing was observed in which a few leaders, by flashing, acted as a stimulus to the discharge of the flashes by the others, thus bring- ing about the flashing in unison of the whole group. 7. The adult fireflies were not observed feeding, except the female of Photurus pennsylvanica. She was found to be very voracious, feeding chiefly on the adults of smaller fireflies, but at times she was found to be cannibalistic. 8. The larvae of all species were found to be predaceous, feeding on snails, earthworms and numerous species of insect larvae, especially cutworms. They are at times quite voracious, and it seems evident that they are of considerable economic importance. 9. By means of their hollow mandibles, the larvae eject a portion of the mid-intestinal juices into its host, thereby paralyz- ing it, and later digesting it so that when the food is taken into the body it is in a digested or nearly digested condition. 10. The larval life, at least, in most of our native fireflies, extends over a period of two years, during which time the larvae live on or near the surface of the ground. BIOLOGY OF SOME COMMON LAMPYRID/E. 75 1 1 . All species that were studied hibernate as larvae underneath stones, a short distance under the ground or near the surface, often in specially constructed chambers. 12. Pupation takes place in moist earth under stones, or in specially constructed pupal-cells at the surface of the ground. The pupal cell of Photurus pennsylvanica is made of short, ribbon-like pieces of earth, which the larva masticates and con- structs into a lattice-like dome. 13. The pupae retain the larval light-organs which function throughout the pupal period. This period was found to be from seven to eighteen days according to the species. 14. The adults live for a period of two to four weeks, during which time they deposit their eggs on the ground about the roots of grass and moss. BIBLIOGRAPHY. Annandale, N. 'oo Insect Luminosity. An Aquatic Lampyrid Larva. Proc. Zool. Soc. London, pp. 862-865. Barber, H. S. '05 Note on Phengodes in Vicinity of Washingtcn, D. C. Proc. Ent. Soc. Wash., VII., pp. 196-197. '14 On Interspecific Mating in Phengodes and Inbreeding in Eros. Proc. Ent. Soc. Wash., XVI., pp. 32-34. Bethune, C. J. S. '68 Luminous Larvae. Can. Ent., I., pp. 38-39. Blair, K. G. '15 Luminous Insects. Nature, XCVL, pp. 411-415. Bongardt, J. '04 Zur Biologic unserer Leuchtkafer. Natw. Wochenschr., Jena XIX., pp. 305-310. Dahlgren, U. '17 The Production of Light by Animals — The Fireflies or Lampyrids. Jour. of Franklin Inst., CLXXXIIL, pp. 323-348. Dubois, R. '86 Contribution a 1'etude de la production de la lumiere par les etres vivants. Les Eletreides lumineux. Bull. Soc. Zool. France, IX., pp. 1-275, P^s- I-IX. Fabre, Henri. '13 The Glow-worm. The First User of Anaesthetics. The Century Magazine, LXXXVIL, pp. 105-112. (Translated by A. T. de Mattos.) Haddon, K. 'iS On the Methods of Feeding and the Mouthparts of the Larva of the Glow- worm (Lampyris noctiiuca). Proc. Zool. Soc. London, pp. 77-82, pi. i. Hudson, G. V. '91 The Habits and Life-history of the New Zealand Glow-worm. Trans, and Proc. New Zeal. Inst., XXIII. , pp. 43-47. pi- VIII. 76 WALTER N. HESS. Knab, F. '05 Observations on Lampyridae. Can. Ent., XXXVII., pp. 238-239. LeConte, J. L. '81 Synopsis of the Lampyridae of the United States. Trans. Am. Ent. Soc., IX., pp. 15-72. Mast, S. O. '12 Behavior of the Fireflies (Photinus pyr'alis) ? with Special Reference to the Problem of Orientation. Jour. Anim. Behav., II., pp. 256-272. McDermott, F. A. '10 A Note on the Light-emission of some American Lampyridae. Can. Ent., XLIL, pp. 237-363. 'n Some Further Observations on the Light-emission of American Lampyridae: the Photogenic Function as a Mating Adaptation in the Photinini. Can. Ent., XLIIL, pp. 399-406. '12 Observations on the Light-emission of American Lampyridae. Can. Ent., XLIV., pp. 309-312. '14 Ecologic Relations of Photogenic Function Among Insects. Zeit. f. wiss. Insecten Biol., X., pp. 303-307. '16 Flashing of Fireflies. Science, XLIY., p. 610. Morse, E. S. '16 Firefly Flashing in LTnison. Science, K.S., XLIIL, pp. 169-170; XLYI , PP- 387-388. Needham, J. G. '03 Button-bush Insects. Psyche, X., pp. 22-30. Newport, G. '57 On the Natural History of the Glow-worm (Lampyris noctilnca). Jour. Proc. Linn. Soc. Zool. London, I., pp. 40-71. Olivier, E. 'n Contribution a 1'histoire des Lampyridje. Mem. Congr. Internat. Brussels (l9IO), II., pp. 273-282, 2 CUtS. Vogel, R. '12 Beitrage zur Anatomic und Biologie der Larva von Lampyris noctilnca. Zool. Anz., XXXIX., pp. 515-519. '15 Beitrage zur Kenntniss des Baues und der Lebensweise der Larve von Lampyris noctiluca. Zeit. f. wiss. Zool., CVIL, pp. 291-432. 35 text figures, pi. 4. Wenzel, H. W. '96 Notes on Lampyridae, with the Description of a Female and Larva. Ent. News, VII., pp. 294-296, pi. XI. Williams, F. X. '17 Notes on the Life-history of some North American Lampyridae. Jour. N. Y. Ent. Soc , XXV., pp. 11-33. AN EXCEPTION TO BATESON'S RULE OF SECOND- ARY SYMMETRY. A. B. DAWSON, DEPARTMENT OF ANATOMY, LOYOLA UNIVERSITY SCHOOL OF MEDICINE. In the summer of 1917, while employed by the Biological Board of Canada in a Dominion Lobster Hatchery at Bay View, N. S., my attention was called to an abnormal lobster caught by a local fisherman in the adjoining waters of Pictou Harbor. The lobster was a male and measured 7>^ inches from rostrum to telson. The abnormality consisted of a double extra claw on the right cheliped, resulting in a condition of incomplete tripli- cation. The presence of the extra parts on the right side did not appear to greatly handicap the animal which, when placed in one of the hatchery tanks, moved about freely. Other abnor- malities somewhat similar have been described for the lobster by Faxon ('81), Emmel ('07) and Cole ('10). DESCRIPTION. The "triple" chela of the right side consisted of a small "nip- per" and a double extra "crusher," while the corresponding appendage of the left side was large, normally developed and of the nipping type. The small "nipper" of the monster claw apparently represented the primary member of the group and, with the exception of the meropodite (M.), was normal in all its segments. Arising from the posterior (morphologically ventral) surface of this meropodite was the double extra claw. The first segment of the right chela, the ischiopodite (Is.} was not exceptionally large and presented no evidence of dis- tortion; but in the meropodite, as already noted, the effect of the abnormality in the appendage was very evident. This segment was of normal width proximally but broadened out rapidly distally and terminated in two diverging branches (M., M.' + ")• On M was borne the small primary claw while M.' + " carried the much larger abnormal structure. 77 7o A. B. DAWSON. On examining the more posterior prong of the compound meropodite, we find that the next distal segment, i.e., the extra carpopodite (C. ' + ") is morphologically double. It is more than twice as large as the corresponding segment (C.) of the primary claw and bears on it supper surface two groups of spines separated by a shallow longitudinal groove. On the end of the massive carpopodite is the large double protopodite (P. ' + "). This segment is incompletely divided, with the separation extending only as far as the region on a level with the bases of D. I. D" FIG. i. (One half natural size.) Male lobster possessing an abnormal right chela consisting of a primary nipping claw plus a double extra "crusher." C., primary carpopodite; C.' * " , double extra carpopodite; D., primary dactyl; D.', D.", two extra dactyls; /., primary index; /.', /.", two extra indices; Is., ischiopodite; M., primary portion of meropodite; M .' + ", extra portion of meropodite; O., conical protuberance on double protopodite; P., primary proto- podite; P' + " (P omitted in reproduction), double extra protopodite; S., scar on left chela. the indices (/. ', /.")• Opposing the indices are two well de- veloped movable dactyls (D/, D."). The two partially sepa- rated chelae are almost exact mirror images of each other. The dactyls and indices are practically identical and the dentition of both consists of heavy tubercle-like teeth. There are no tactile hairs. Each segment of the double extra claw was decidedly larger than the corresponding portion of the primary claw, but the entire "triple" appendage barely equalled the left nipping claw in weight. The long axes of the double appendage and primary EXCEPTION TO BATESON's RULE OF SECONDARY SYMMETRY. 79 appendage were in one plane and all three dactyls moved to meet their opposing indices in this plane. Another point of considerable interest is the color relation of the extra portions. In the double extra protopodite and the two extra dactyls the pigmentation was completely reversed, giving as a result a light colored upper and a densely pigmented lower surface. A peculiar conical protuberance present on the light upper surface of the double protopodite was practically free from pigment. THEORETICAL CONSIDERATIONS. The present example, like most abnormal crustacean appen- dages, falls into the category established by Bateson ('94), "in which the extra limb or extra parts of a limb are themselves morphologically double," but unlike the others it does not conform to the rules of secondary symmetry formulated by the same author. According to these rules the normal appendage and the extra parts lie in the same plane and "the nearer of the two extra appendages is in structure and position formed as the image of the normal appendage in a plane mirror placed between the normal appendage and the nearer one, at right angles to the plane of the three axes; and the remoter appendage is the image of the nearer in a plane mirror similarly placed between the two extra appendages" (p. 479). In the case under discussion the pair of extra chelae are mirror images of each other but the appendage nearer the primary claw is not, as the rules provide, a mirror image of the latter. More- over, it does not appear possible to explain this exception to the rules of secondary symmetry by a reference to torsion. Emmel ('07) and Cole ('10) were unable to explain apparent exceptions to Bateson's rules by making allowances for changes in position due to possible torsion. It has previously been pointed out that the coloration of the abnormal double structure was completely reversed, but even if the extra segments were rotated so that the dark surface became uppermost the relations of the claws as regards secondary symmetry would remain unchanged since the double extra claw is bilaterally symmetrical. The specimen here described while exhibiting many points of 8O A. B. DAWSON. resemblance to the abnormal chelipeds figured by Emmel ('07, PI. 2, Fig. 5) and Cole ('10, Figs, i, 2) also presents several novel features. The degree of "triplication" of the claw is less than that seen in Emmel's specimen but greater than that de- scribed by Cole. In the former, the abnormal processes, con- sisting of a double carpopodite, two protopodites and two dactyls arose from the meropodite. In the latter, the abnormal struc- ture, two extra indices and a double extra dactyl, was borne on the normal protopodite. In Faxon's specimen ('81) the mor- phological character of the extra branch which is borne on the forked meropodite is questionable. Faxon himself does not believe that the structure is double but Bateson ( '94) is inclined to regard it as being morphologically double. Furthermore both Emmel and Cole found that the conditions in their "triple" claws illustrate the rules of secondary symmetry almost diagram- matically, i.e., when allowance was made for shifting due to torsion. In the two chelae described by Emmel and Cole the two extra claws were of the same character as the primary claw, "crushers." In my specimen the primary claw is a "nipper" while the two extra claws are of the crushing type with well-developed tubercle- like teeth. This condition is of special significance, when we recall that the claw of the opposite side is also of the nipping type. In other words, we have a lobster with the "great" claws symmetrical with reference to each other, besides bearing on the meropodite of the right a double extra "crusher." Emmel ('07) finds the pigmentation reversed on one of the extra claws but is able to explain this abnormal condition by a reference to torsion. The case which I am describing does not admit of such an explanation. The cause of the abnormality is unknown. No scars were to be found on the "triple" claw, but on the protopodite of the appendage of the opposite side a definite scar (S) was present. There were no further evidences of mutilation. The results obtained by Harrison ('17) with transplantation of limb buds in Amblystoma larvae suggest that there may possibly be some direct relation between the reversal of pigmentation and the doubling of the extra appendage. Harrison found that in EXCEPTION TO BATESON's RULE OF SECONDARY SYMMETRY. 8 1 transplantations in which the limb buds were inverted, a certain percentage gave rise to double or twin limbs, one being a mirror image of the other. In a few cases there were still further duplications so that more or less complete triple limbs resulted, having approximately the same relations as found by Bateson in the Arthropoda. In the lobster just described, two facts were noted; first, the extra parts are double and second, their pigmentation is reversed indicating an inversion of the double portion of the triple appen- dage. Furthermore, it is generally conceded that most abnormal and duplicated appendages among Crustacea are the result of regenerative processes. Both Reed ( '04) and Emmel ( '07) have found that abnormalities can be produced experimentally by mutilating either the proximal stump or the developing limb bud. It is conceivable then that the triplication found in the present instance may be due to regeneration following injury. In the course of regeneration the growing but may have been also injured so as to cause the development of an extra process (Emmel, '07, pp. 114-115), and this process may have had its dorso-ventral orientation reversed either at the time its develop- ment was initiated or at some later date. Keeping the above statements in mind it therefore becomes possible to elaborate a more or less satisfactory hypothetical explanation of the triple chela. First, there may have been an injury to a normally developed crushing claw, followed by autot- omy. Later the developing bud may have been injured, resul- ting in the appearance of an extra bud on the surface of the primary one. As a consequence of the injury, the primary bud did not develop as a crushing claw but as a small "nipper." (Sufficient evidence has been adduced by Emmel in 1907, Figs. 24 to 31, to prove that abnormal symmetrical claws in lobsters do arise through regeneration following mutilation.) The extra bud, due to mechanical displacement of its tissue at the time of the first injury, or as a result of a subsequent accident, has had its dorso-ventral orientation reversed, causing the develop- ment of twin crushing claws which are mirror images of each other. 82 A. B. DAWSON. SUMMARY. 1. An abnormal lobster cheliped is described in this paper. The abnormality consists of a double extra crushing claw arising from the meropodite of a claw of the nipping type. 2. The conditions found here are shown to present an exception to Bateson's rules of secondary symmetry. 3. The primary member of the so-called triple claw was found to be of the same character as the corresponding appendage of the opposite side, furnishing an exception to the normal condition of asymmetry. 4. Pigmentation was reversed in the double extra claw. 5. There is some evidence in favor of the view that there is a definite relation between the reversal of pigmentation and the doubling of the extra appendage. LITERATURE. Bateson, W. '94 Materials for the Study of Variation, Treated with Especial Regard to Discontinuity in the Origin of Species. London. Cole, L. J. '10 Description of an Abnormal Lobster Cheliped. BIOLOGICAL BULLETIN, Vol. 18, pp. 252-268. 9 figs. Emmel, V. E. '07 Regenerated and Abnormal Appendages in the Lobster. _3?th Annual Report Com. of Inland Fisheries, R. I., pp. 99-152, PI. 1-9. Faxon, W. '81 On Some Crustacean Deformities. Bull, of Mus. Comp. Zool., Vol. 8, No. 13, PP- 257-275, Pis. 1-2. Harrison, R. G. '17 Transplantation of Limbs. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci., pp. 245-251. Reed, Margaret A. '04 The Regeneration of the First Leg of the Crayfish. Arch. Entw.-Mech. Bd. 18, p. 307. A COMPARATIVE STUDY OF THE CHROMOSOMES OF LACHNOSTERNA (COLEOPTERA). E. L. SHAFFER, DEPARTMENT OF BIOLOGY, PRINCETON UNIVERSITY. A. INTRODUCTION. The studies presented here were begun in the spring of 1916 at Princeton University and were continued until the spring of 1917. Most of the material used was collected at Cold Spring Harbor during the summer of 1916. l The studies were discontinued in the summer of 1917 owing to the war. On returning to Princeton last spring, it was thought advisable to assemble the observations previously made despite the fact that they did not represent as complete a study as had been intended. I wish to express my sincere thanks to Professor E. G. Conklin for much valuable assistance and encouragement. B. MATERIALS AND PLAN OF STUDY. It was originally intended to make a detailed study of the process of synapsis as well as a comparative study of the chro- mosomes of four selected species of May beetles, genus Lach- nosterna. While the material was not entirely favorable for these purposes, some interesting facts were brought to light. The four scarab beetles of the genus Lachnosterna which were selected for study were the species delata, fusca, gracilis and tristis. Besides these, for comparative purposes, two other scarab beetles were .studied, Pelidonota pimctata and Cotalpa lanigera. The form most studied was L. delata and since the other forms showed no essential differences from delata, the latter will be used as the basis of description in the present paper. Comparatively little detailed study of spermatogenesis in the Coleoptera has been done. The work of Miss Stevens ('05, '06), 1 The writer wishes to express his thanks to the Brooklyn Institute of Arts anP Sciences for the privileges of a research fellowship at the Laboratory of the Institute at Cold Spring Harbor, L. L, during the summer of 1916. 83 84 E. L. SHAFFER. while it covered a large number of species, was concerned only with chromosome counts, and especially with reference to the sex chromosomes. In only two species of beetles has there been anything like a detailed study of the chromosomes in synapsis, Voinov (1903) on Cybister roselii and Schafer (1907) on Dytiscus marginalis. Both these authors after a detailed study of the growth stages of the spermatocytes describe parasynapsis, while Miss Stevens claims telosynapsis in the forms she studied. FIG. i. Single testis of Lachnosterna (side-View), with its efferent duct. The material used in this study was fixed in Flemming's, Her- mann's, Bouin's, Gilson's and Carnoy's fixing fluids. In general, the Flemming and the Hermann material was best for the growth stages of the spermatocytes, while the Bouin material was best for the chromosomes. Iron-haematoxylin, with and without a counterstain, was employed entirely for staining. Aceto-carmine smears were valuable in checking the observations on the fixed material. All the testes, except those of L. ftisca, were taken v from the adult beetles. The material was gathered in midsummer and showed all stages from spermatogonia to ripe spermatozoa. In the case of L. fusca, the adult testes showed few favorable stages and it was necessary to study the larval gonads. C. DESCRIPTION OF TESTES AND SERIATION OF STAGES. The testes consist of twelve mushroom-like bodies, three pair in each side of the abdomen. Each testis has its duct (Figs. i and 2) and the ducts from each group of testes unite to form two larger ducts; these four larger ducts in turn unite to form the single median vas deferens. STUDY OF CHROMOSOMES OF LACHNOSTERNA. 85 The testes, although of an unusual shape, show the seriation of the stages clearly, being but a modification of the simple straight (orthopteran) type with a linear seriation of the cells. In the species studied, the testes consist of a great many follicles radiating from the center. Fig. 2 represents a diagrammatic section through the center of the testis and perpendicular to its broad surface. In the center of the testis from which the follicles radiate (Fig. 2, A), one finds all the spermatogonia and here new cysts are in the process of formation. On each side of this FIG. 2. Diagrammatic section through testis at right angles to its broad surface, to show seriation of stages. .4, region in which are all spermatogonia; B, early growth stages and synezesis; C, pacyhtene and later growth stages; D, maturation divisions; £, spermatids and spermatozoa; F, cavity in testis where spermatozoa are retained prior to discharge from testis; G, efferent duct. region (B, B} are the early synaptic stages (synezesis). In the regions C-C one finds the later synaptic stages (pachytene and diplotenes), while in regions D-D show most of the spermatocyte divisions; regions E-E contain most of the spermatids and spermatozoa. The chamber (F) shown in the figure serves as a place where the spermatozoa are collected and stored prior to discharge from the testis; the duct (G) leads from the storage chamber. Of course the stages above seriated overlap and there is no sharp delimitation as is diagrammatically shown in the figure. The formation of the cysts in the region A was followed out and my observations confirm those of Wieman ('10) and Hegner ('14) that each testicular cyst is derived from a single spermatogonium. There is, however, no evidence that cell division is by amitosis as Wieman found. 86 E. L. SHAFFER. D. OBSERVATIONS. i. Spermatogonia and Diploid Chromosome Groups. In all of the species of Lachnosterna studied, as well as Peli- donota and Cotalpa, the diploid number of chromosomes as shown in the spermatogonia is twenty, including an unequal (sex) pair (Figs. 1-6). Dividing follicle cells in the ovaries show ten equal pairs of chromosomes (Fig. 7). There are three pairs of J- or U-shaped chromosomes, one pair of which is considerably larger than the others (Figs. 1-5, A A). The sex chromosomes are the smallest in the complex, consisting of a very small round chro- mosome (;y) and a somewhat larger rod-shaped chromosome (x). In comparing the size relations of the chromosomes in the several species studied, one finds no marked differences. In many cases the chromosomes in the diploid complexes are arranged in pairs, homologous chromosomes lying beside each other. In the Diptera, Metz (1916) has found that pairing of chromosomes is not confined to the maturation stages, but at each cell division homologous chromosomes come together. In the Diptera the diploid chromosome number is relatively low; in species where the. chromosome number is high, pairing of homo- logous chromosomes is usually not found to be so complete. It therefore seems that chromosome pairing, outside of the matur- ation stages, is related to chromosome number. In the spermatogonial telophases, the chromosomes spin out into fine chromatic threads (Fig. 13) and as the nucleus grows the threads become more and more complex forming a chromatic reticulum or typical resting nucleus. This "resting" nucleus is of relatively short duration, for soon the chromatin begin to condense into heavier threads (Fig. 15), and as condensation continues, all the chromatin of the nucleus becomes confined into large chromatic blocks of a granular nature (Figs. 16, 17). Counts of these chromatic blocks in uncut nuclei always approxi- mate the diploid chromosome number and these blocks may be considered as the anlages of the future spermatogonial chromo- somes. The blocks consist of a linin-core on which are imbedded the chromatin granules; they are connected to each other by a fine net-work of linin which seems to be continuous with the linin forming the core of the blocks. Most of the cells in the spermato- STUDY OF CHROMOSOMES OF LACHNOSTERNA. 87 gonial area of the testes are in this stage and apparently it is of of much longer duration than the reticular or "resting" stage. In some respects the chromatic blocks above described cor- respond to the " prochromosomes " which -have been described by Overton ('09) in Podophyllum, Arnold ('08) in Hydrophilus piceus, Goodrich ('16) in Ascaris incurva and other workers. In these cases, however, the chromatic bodies appeared at the beginning of the growth period and, according to the above workers, these bodies arranged themselves in pairs, thereby accomplishing the synaptic process. In Lachno sterna there is no such paired arrangement of the chromatic blocks; they merely represent stages in the formation of the spermatogonial chro- mosome groups and might really be called prophases, except that they are of relatively long duration. In some cases a precocious longitudinal split can be detected, preparing the chromosome for the next cell division. 2. The Synaptic Stages and Maturation Divisions. Following the telophase of the last spermatogonial division (Fig. 13), the chromosomes spin out in the form of very fine (leptotene] threads (Fig. 18, 19) which entirely fill the nucleus and prevent a minute analysis of this stage. The actual pairing of the homologous chromosomes could not be followed in detail, but observations on a few favorable cells (Fig. 19) indicate that the union is side-to-side (parasynapsis) . Stevens ('06) has described telosynapsis in the Coleoptera, but she did not make a study of the early growth stages. The leptotene stage gradually merges into a definite contrac- tion stage (synezesis) with all the chromatic threads polarized at one side of the nucleus (Figs. 20, 44). These stages are always found in a definite part of the testes, namely in region B (Fig. 2), and are found nowhere else. McClung ('05) used the word "synezesis" to describe that "condition of the nucleus in which the chromatin is found massed at one side of the vesicle, without regard to whether it is a normal phenomenon or not." McClung and recently some of his students, Whiting ('17) and Hance ('17), have maintained that a unilateral massing of the chromatin or synezesis is an artifact and is due to improper 88 E. L. SHAFFER. fixation methods. With this in mind, the writer has sought with most careful technical methods to obtain fixed material which might not show these contraction figures; but without exception cells in the contraction phase were always found in the definite region of the testes mentioned above. It is quite true that poorly fixed material shows an abundance of contraction figures, but in these cases, as will be shown later, they are just as likely to appear in other regions of the testes than in the very definite location above mentioned. There is no doubt that even the best fixation will tend to emphasize the contraction of the chromatin just as it does in the case of the other cell structures, but synezesis is unquestionably a normal process in the beetles studied here. Whiting ('17) has advanced the idea that the chromatic elements during synapsis are in an unstable condition and that "any shock is likely to cause them to clump together." It is questionable whether good fixation is much less of a " shock" than indifferent fixation. It is conceivable that true synezesis may not occur as a normal phase of the maturation possesses in some animals (e.g., Orthoptera), but the fact that it has been described by many workers using a variety of fixing methods supports the fact that it is a normally occurring phase in some cases. In any event it proves that the nuclear condition is peculiar in cases of synezesis. Following the stage of synezesis, the chromatin threads are released from the polarized bouquet in the form of thick ragged looking pachytene threads (Fig. 21). Usually a longitudinal split can be seen in the threads, which marks the point of synapsis of the homologous threads. The chromomeres are imbedded in a linin base, chromomeres of the same size lying opposite each other and being connected with each other by fine linin threads. In the later stages the threads become more widely separated from each other (Fig. 23) assuming the diplotene form. In these stages and in still later ones, the threads show a variety of twisting about each other forming rings with and without crossed ends, figures 8, double and even triple crossing-over of the threads. In no case could a secondary split be seen. The strepsistene threads continue to become more widely separated and it soon becomes impossible to trace the individual threads (Fig. 22). STUDY OF CHROMOSOMES OF LACHNOSTERNA. 89 This unanalyzable stage is of relatively short duration and is followed by a condensation of the chromatin in the form of heavy threads (Fig. 25). Condensation of the chromatin continues and the definitive maturation tetrads begin to make their appear- ance. In these early prophases one often finds cells in which all the chromatin is massed at one side of the nucleus (Fig. 26), resembling very much a synezesis figure. Gross ('07) has described a second synezesis in Pyrrhocoris and Mottier ( '07) be- lieves that in the plants it is a regularly occurring phase in the maturation processes. In Lachnosterna these contraction figures are most abundant in material which is poorly fixed and I consider them as artifacts. Fig. 27 represents this stage from well fixed material as contrasted with Fig. 26 from poorly fixed material. All the first spermatocyte metaphase plates of the four species of Lachnosterna studied, as well as Pelidonota and Cotalpa, show ten bivalent chromosomes the smallest of which represents the sex pair (Figs. 8, 9, 10, n, 12). These are usually arranged in characteristic groups with fine linin threads connecting the various members of the complex to each other. A comparison of the tetrads of L. delata with those of L. fusca (Figs. 28, 29) shows no marked differences either in form or in size of the tetrads. Using Miss Carothers' ('17) nomenclature, there are five atelomitic tetrads (non-terminal spindle fiber attachments) and five telom- itic tetrads (terminal spindle fiber attachments). The atelom- itic tetrads are the largest in the complex and are derived from the three pairs of J-shaped spermatogonial chromosomes and two pairs of the bent rod-shaped ones. In sideview metaphases, the largest of the tetrads (Figs. 28, 29) has a sub-terminal spindle fiber attachment, and is derived from the AA pair (Fig. 5) of the diploid chromosome group which also have sub- terminal fiber attachments. The other atelomitic tetrads consist of two typical crosses and two annular tetrads 'of the Stenobothrus type. The other four autosome tetrads are of the ordinary dumb-bell type while the x and y elements (sex pair) are fused end to end (Figs. 28, 29) , The types of tetrads above described are found in all four species of Lachnosterna studied. On the other hand, in Cotalpa 9O E. L. SHAFFER. and Pelidonota no cross-shaped tetrads and only one ring tetrad are found. The question of reduction division is difficult to analyze here, with the exception of the ring tetrads. The latter are always arranged on the spindle in the direction of the spindle axis and the spindle fiber attachment is median. Consequently the separation of the dyads occurrs at the point of the synaptic union and the division is reductional. 3. Sex Chromosomes. The earliest work on the sex chromosomes was done by Miss Stevens ('05, '06) on the Coleoptera. She found the so-called sex chromosomes in over forty species and her work and that of Wilson's on the Hemiptera and McClung's on the Orthoptera have been the basis of the later work correlating sex determina- tion with the chromosomes. In the Coleoptera the sex chro- mosomes are found as unpaired "accessory" and as unequal elements which separate in one of the maturation divisions and divide equationally in the other maturation division. Arnold ('08) has maintained that in Hydrophilus piceus there are no sex chromosomes. There is present in the growth stages a chromatin nucleolus which may even persist up to the first maturation division and may even pass undivided to one cell. However, it disappears and cannot be found in any of the second spermatocytes. In Ladmosterna, Pelidonota and Cotalpa the sex chromosomes are of the xy type the y element being the smaller of the unequal pair (Figs. 1-6). There are no marked differences in the size and form of the sex chromosomes in the four species of Lach- nosterna studied, but in Pelidonota the x element is considerably larger than in the Lachnosterna material. In all cases the sex pair separate in the first maturation division and divide equa- tionally in the second, thus yielding two types of spermatozoa. (Figs. 31, 32, 33, 35, 36). In a single case the sex chromosomes failed to separate in the first maturation division, both chromo- somes going into one of the daughter cells. This is undoubtedly a case of non-disjunction similar to that which has been found genetically and cytologically by Bridges ('16) in Drosophila. The sex chromosomes presist throughout the entire growth STUDY OF CHROMOSOMES OF LACHNOSTERNA. QI period as definite compact chromatic bodies. They are always contained within a chromosomal vesicle such as has been de- scribed by Wilson ('12) in Oncopeltus and Lygeeus (Figs. 21, 25). In Lachnosterna the sex elements usually remain separate from each other, each enclosed in a separate vesicle. In Pelidonota and Cotalpa, the sex pair remain fused during the synaptic period, the smaller (y) element usually being imbedded along the side of the larger (x) element. E. GENERAL CONSIDERATIONS. i. Chromosome Number and Species. The intensive work of McClung and his students on one family of Orthoptera, the Acrididse, has shown that the chromosome number in all the species studied of this group is the same, namely 23 in the male. This has led McClung to the generali- zation that species closely related taxonomically might show similarity in their chromosome groups. It is very evident that this generalization cannot apply to all groups since, in some cases there is a wide divergence in chromosome number among members of the same genus. It is possible that in some cases this difference in chromosome number between closely related species may be due to a fusion of several chromosomes or else a breaking up of one or more chromosomes into several distinct components. In the case of Hesperotettix, McClung ('17) has shown that the chromosome number may vary from 17 to 23. He has shown that these variations are due to a fusion of chromo- somes resulting in the formation of "multiple chromosomes." In one species, Hesperotettix viridis, he has found the haploid or reduced number to vary from 9 to 13. On the other hand, the work of Stevens on the Diabrolicas (Coleoptera) has shown that the species vitatta has 21 chromosomes, while the species soror and 12-punctata have but 19. However, in the latter two species there may be present from i to 4 additional or "supernumary " small chromosomes. It is quite possible that the supernumary chromosomes of the species soror and 12-punctata represent the fragments of a pair of chromosomes, which would therefore make an agreement in chromosome number between these two species and the species vitatta. As McClung ('17, p. 545) has pointed 92 E. L. SHAFFER. out, he has confined his idea of this similarity of chromosome number in closely related species, only to the family Acrididse. It is possible that in other forms correspondence in chromosomes may extend only to the subfamily or genus. In the Hemiptera and Coleoptera certainly there is no such uniformity of chrom- osome number in the various families as is found in the Acrididse. The four species of Lachnosterna studied here differ from each other very much as far as taxonomic characters are concerned, nevertheless the chromosome groups show no difference either in form or number. The two other forms studied, Pelidonota and Cotalpa, differing generically, have the same chromosome number (20 in the diploid groups), but there are some differences in the form of the maturation tetrads. Only one other scarab bettle has been studied, Euphoria inda by Stevens ('06), and it corresponds with a diploid group of 20 chromosomes, so that all the species of the family Scarabidce thus far studied correspond in chromosome number. The genus Lachnosterna embraces over one hundred species, some very much alike so that it is difficult to separate them taxonomically, others differing markedly from each other. The most constant difference is found in the male copulatory organs, which probably prevents the inter- breeding of species in nature. Perhaps further cytological studies in this genus will yield results similar to those in the Acrididse. Certainly there is a wealth of material for such a comparative study. 2. Cyst Formation and Cell Polarity. Hegner ('14) has studied the formation of spermatogonial cysts in the testes of Leptinotarsa; the facts concerning cyst formation in the beetles studied here show results essentially similar to those of Hegner. The primary spermatogonia are not arranged in cysts and are more or less polygonal in shape, with the nucleus usually located in the center. Cyst formation begins by the rapid division of a single primary spermatogonium, together with an adjacent epithelial cell which forms a follicular membrane around the cyst. Consequently we can say that all the cells within any one cyst are the descendants of a single primary spermatogonium. With the formation of the cyst, STUDY OF CHROMOSOMES OF LACHNOSTERNA. 93 the spermatogonia are arranged in the form of a rosette, and are now triangular or wedge-shaped with the nucleus at the base and the rest of the cytoplasm extending toward the cyst cavity. Thus, with the formation of the cyst, there is a polarity estab- lished in the spermatogonia which is maintained up to the for- mation of the ripe spermatozoa, for, the side where the nucleus is located is destined to form the head of the spermatozoon, and the cytoplasmic portion extending toward the cyst cavity is destined to form its tail. Hegner. ('14) has homologized the process of cyst formation with the differential divisions in insect oogenesis which establish nurse cells and oocytes. It has long- been known that the insect egg possesses a remarkable polarity besides being highly organized. Since, as it has been above shown, the polarity of the sperm cells are established at the time of cyst formation, and since this process is homologous to nurse cell-oocyte differentiation, it is probable that the polarity of the egg may have its origin at the time of the differentiation of nurse cells and oocytes. 3. Linin and Chromosome Structure. When one studies the history of the chromatin of the nucleus from the resting stage through the synaptic period up to the reconstitution of the definitive maturation chromosomes, one begins to seek for some of the underlying mechanisms concerned in the movements of the chromatin particles. From the diffuse granular state of the chromatin up until the formation of the chromosomes, the linin network of the nucleus plays an active part. Chromatin granules in the nucleus are never isolated as such, but always have linin connections with other granules. The synaptic threads consist of linin threads with the chro- momeres embedded along them. As has been before stated, homologous chromomeres have linin connections running between them. Conklin ('17) has shown that the ground-work of the cytoplasm is the relatively stable and elastic spongioplasm, and he attributes to it the maintenance of cytoplasmic organization and the movements and localization of cytoplasmic substances. Similarly in the nucleus it seems that the linin is a relatively elastic substance which forms the ground-work of the nucleus 94 E. L. SHAFFER. and maintains the organization of the nuclear elements. Wenrich ('16) has shown how remarkably constant the organization and "architecture" of the chromosomes are. By means of certain structural peculiarities which his "selected" chromosomes presented, he was able to recognize and trace them through all the stages of spermatogenesis. The tendency has been noted in many forms for the chromosomes to appear in the metaphase always in a definite configuration. It is possible that the linin connections between the chromosomes which have often been figured (Figs. 8-12) are responsible for the definite patterns as- sumed by the chromosomes in the metaphase plate. The uniting in pairs of the homologous leptotene threads may be due to the contractility of the linin connectives running between the homologous chromomeres. In short, the morphological stability of the nuclear elements and the constancy of their form, arrangement and organization is in the last analysis referable to the linin. F. SUMMARY. 1. The diploid chromosome groups of four species of Lach- nosterna, namely delala, fusca, gracilis and tristis, as well as Pelidonota punctata and Cotalpa lanigera, show twenty chromo- somes, one pair of which is composed of two unequal elements (sex chromosomes) . 2. There are no essential differences in the form and arrange- ment of the chromosomes in the species studied. 3. The growth period of the spermatocytes is marked by the appearance of delicate leptotene threads which are derived from the chromosomes of the last spermatogonial division. These threads become polarized and there is evidence that they are arranged in pairs parasynaptically. 4. There is a definite contraction stage which does not seem to be caused by fixation, but is a normally occurring phase in the growth period. 5. The sex chromosomes persist through the entire growth- period in the form of definite compact bodies, sometimes being contained within chromosomal vesicles. The unequal sex elements separate in the first maturation division and divide equationally in the second maturation division. STUDY OF CHROMOSOMES OF LACHNOSTERNA. 95 6. There are five atelomitic tetrads in the first maturation division and five telomitic tetrads (including the sex pair). 7. Cyst formation in the testis begins by the rapid division of a single primary spermatogonium, so that all the cells within any particular cyst are the descendants of a single cell. The visible polarity of the cells seems to be established at the time of cyst formation. G. ADDENDA. Since the manuscript of the foregoing study was written, the work of Goldsmith1 has appeared on the chromosomes of the Cicindelidae. He has studied in all five species of this family and finds that they agree in chromosome number. He has described a double odd-chromosome which passes undivided to one pole of the spindle in the first maturation division and divides in the second maturation division giving rise to sper- matozoa with ten and twelve chromosomes respectively. In his study of the growth stages of the spermatocytes he has been unable to find that the leptotene threads actually pair. His figures of the synaptic stages are not clear and he makes no decision as to the method of synapsis (parasynapsis or telosyn- apsis). He describes the "early" spermatogonia as being arranged in syncytia without any discernible cell-walls. He describes the appearance within the syncytial cytoplasm of "cytoplasmic fibrillar bridges." "With the increase in age and size of the cells, these bridges become more dense and assume a definite arrangement about a number of cells. This continues until the entire tubule is subdivided into a large number of syncytia— cysts containing cells without perceptible cell walls" (p. 445). Both his descriptions and figures of this peculiar method of cyst formation lack in clarity. From his Fig. 5, I interpret the "cytoplasmic fibrillar bridges "as being probably the persisting spindle remains or mitosome of the previous division, and it is also probable that the deeply staining bodies in the cytoplasm are the mid-bodies (cell-plate) persisting with the mitosome. That these "fibrillar bridges" are really spindle remains is further 1A comparative study of the chromosomes of the tiger beetles (Cicindelidae). Jour. Morph., Vol. 32, No. 3, 1919. 96 E. L. SHAFFER. indicated by the fact that they" become more dense and assume a definite arrangement about a number of cells." This is exactly the behavior of the spindle remains which Hegner1 has described in Leptinotarsa and which I have described in Passalus.2 It is difficult to see how these "fibrillar bridges" are concerned in dividing the syncytia into a number of cysts. Furthermore, it is difficult to believe that a true syncytium of spermatogonia actually does exist, for the later stages certainly do possess cell-walls which must have been preexisting. LITERATURE CITED. Arnold, G. '08 The Nucleolus and Michrochromosomes in the Spermatogenesis of Hydro- philus piceus. Arch. f. Zellforsch., Bd. 2. Bridges, C. B. '16 Non-disjunction as a Proof of the Chromosome Theory of Heredity. Genetics, Vol. i. Carothers, E. E. '17 The Segregation and Recombination of Homologous Chromosomes as Found in Two Genera of Acrididae (Orthoptera). Jour. Morph., Vol. 28, No. 2. Conklin, E. G. '17 Effects of Centrifugal Force on the Structure and Development of the Eggs of Crepidula. Jour. Exp. Zool., Vol. 22, No. 2. Goodrich, H. B. '16 The Germ Cells in Ascaris incurva. Jour. Exp. Zool., Vol. 21, No. i. Gross, J. '07 Die spermatogenese von Pyrrhocoris apterus L. Zool. Jahrb Abtheil. f. Ariat. u. Ontog., Bd. 23. Hegner, R. W. '14 The Germ Cell Cycle in Animals. Macmillans. Metz, C. W. '16 Chromosomes Studies on Diptera, II. Jour. Exp. Zool., Vol. 21, No. 2. Mottier, D. M. '07 The Development of Heterotype Chromosomes in Pollen Mother Cells. Ann. of Bot., Vol. XXL McClung, C. E. '05 The Chromosome Complex of Orthopteran Spermatocytes. BIOL. BULL., Vol. IX., No. s- '17 The Multiple Chromosomes of Hesperotettix and Mermiria (Orthoptera). Jour. Morph., Vol. 29. Overton, J. B. '09 On the Organization of the Nuclei in the Pollen Mother Cells of Certain Plants with Especial Reference to the Permanence of the Chromosomes. Arch. f. Mikr. Anat., Bd. 70. 1 Studies on the Germ Cells, I and II. Jour. Morph., Vol. 25, No. 3, 1914. 2 Mitochondria and Other Cytoplasmic Structures in the Spermatogenesis of Passulus cornutus. BIOL. BULL., Vol. XXIL, No. 5, 1917. STUDY OF CHROMOSOMES OF LACHNOSTERNA. 97 Schafer, F. '07 Spermatogenese von Dytiscus. Zool. Jahrb. Abtheil. f. Anat. u. Ontog., Bd. 23. Stevens, N. M. '06 Studies on Spermatogenesis, II. Carnegie Inst. Pub. No. 36. '05 Studies in Spermatogenesis with Especial Reference to the Accessory Chromosome. Carnegie Inst. Pub. No. 36. '08 The chromosomes in Diabrotica vitatta, Diabrotica soror and Diabrotica i2-punctata. Jour. Exp. Zool., Vol. V., No. 4. '09 Further Studies on the Chromosomes of the Coleoptera. Jour. Exp. Zool., Vol. VI., No. i. Voinov, D. '03 La spermatogenese d'ete chez le Cybister roeselii. Arch, de Zool. Exp. et Gen., 4e Series, T. i. Wenrich, D. H. '16 The Spermatogenesis of Phyronotettix magnus with Especial Reference to Synapsis and the Individuality of the Chromosomes. Bull. Mus. Comp. Zool., Harvard Coll., Vol. IX., No. 3. Whiting, P. W. '17 The Chromosomes of the Common House-mosquito, Culex pipiens. Jour. Morph., Vol. 28, No. 2. Wilson, E. B. '12 Studies on the Chromosomes, VIII. Jour. Exp. Zool., Vol. 13, No. 3. Wieman, H. L. '10 A Study in the Germ Cells of Leptinotarsa signaticollis . Jour. Morph., Vol. 21. q8 E. L. SHAFFER. EXPLANATION OF PLATES. All drawings (except Figs. 28, 29, 30) were made with camera lucida using Nc. 12 ocular and 1/16 mm. oil immersion objective. Figs. 28, 29, 30 were made using No. 18 ocular and 1/16 mm. oil objective. PLATE I. (Figs, i to 12.) FIGS, i to 6. Metaphase plates of spermatogonia in the six beetles studied all showing 20 chromosomes including an unequal pair (XY). FIG. i. L. delata. FIG. 2. L. fusca. FIG. 3. L. tristis. FIG. 4. L. gracilis. FIG. 5. Pelidonota. FIG. 6. Cotalpa. FIG. 7. Metaphase plate of follicle cell from ovary of L. delata showing ten equal pairs. FIGS. 8 to 12. Metaphase plates of ist spermatocytes showing ten bivalent chromosomes. FIG. 8. L. delata. FIG. 9. L. fusca. FIG. 10. L. gracilis. FIG. ii. Pelidonota. FIG. 12. Cotalpa. BIOLOGICAL BULLETIN, VOL. XXXVIII. PLATE I. 4 A B 6 9 10 11 12 E. L. SHAFFER. IOO E. L. SHAFFER. PLATE II. (Figs. 13 to 27.) FIG. 13. Telophase nucleus of spermatogonium, showing chromosomes spinning out into delicate threads. FIG. 14. Characteristic resting nucleus of spermatogonium. FIGS. 15, 16, 17. Stages in the condensation of the chromatin from the resting stage to the formation of the chromatic blocks. FIGS. 18, 19. Early growth stages. Evidences of parallel pairing of leptonete threads. FIG. 20. Contraction (synezesis) stage. FIG. 21. Pachytene threads released from synezesis stage. FIG. 22. Strepsistene nucleus. Chromatin threads unanalyzable. FIG. 23. Various forms of diplotene and strepsitene threads. FIG. 25. Early prophase of ist spermatocyte. Tetrads beginning to form. FIG. 26. Cell in prophase simulating synezesis; due to faulty fixation. FIG. 27. Cell in stage similar to Fig. 26 from well-fixed material. BIOLOGICAL BULLETIN, VOL. XXXHII. PLATE II. 13 17 V 21 E. L, SHAFFER. IO2 E. L. SHAFFER. PLATE III. (Figs. 28 to 36.) FIG. 28. Side view of maturation tetrads of L. delata. • FIG. 29. Side view of maturation tetrads of L. fusca. FIG. 30. Side view of maturation tetrads of Pelidonota punctata. FIG. 31. Early anaphase of first maturation division in L. fusca showing separation of sex pair. FIG. 32. Anaphase of first maturation in Pelidonota. FIG. 33. Telophase of first maturation in L. delata. FIG. 34. Telophase of first maturation in L. delata in which the sex elements have failed to disjoin, and have passed to one daughter cell. FIGS. 35, 36. Daughter plates of second spermatocyte of L. delata and Pelido- nota, respectively. BIOLOGICAL BULLETIN. VOL. XXXVIII PIAIE Ml. 4100**"' 28 -. . . . « 29 33 32 35 \ E. L. SHAFFER. Vol. XXXVIII. March, 1920. No. 3. BIOLOGICAL BULLETIN CARBON DIOXIDE PRODUCTION IN RELATION TO REGENERATION IN PLANARIA DOROTOCEPHALA.1 HARRIET L. ROBBINS AND C. M. CHILD. Numerous lines of evidence indicate that the body of Planaria dorotocephala consists physiologically of more than one individual or "zooid" after a certain limit of size is exceeded in the course of growth. The first or chief zooid of the series includes the region from the head to a level slightly posterior to the mouth, the level at which fission usually occurs, and the region posterior to this consists of one or more short zooids, the limits of which can be distinguished physiologically, but not morphologically (Child, '10, nc, '15, chap. VI). The susceptibility of the body to a large number of chemical and physical agents in con- centrations or intensities too high to permit acclimation or tolerance, decreases from the head posteriorly through the length of the first zooid, increases sharply at the level of fission and in long animals shows one or more rises posterior to that level (Child '13^). The process of regeneration also shows graded differences at different levels, corresponding to the dif- ferences in susceptibility (Child 'lib). In the papers referred to, as well as in many others evidence has been presented to that the susceptibility differences in general are in some degree an indicator of quantitative differences in metabolic condition, particularly in rate of oxidations, though susceptibility has never been regarded as an exact quantitative measure of oxidation. According to this point of view the gradations of susceptibility in the body of Planaria dorotoceph- ala indicate that the rate of oxidation decreases gradually, at 1 From the Hull Zoological Laboratory, University of Chicago. 103 104 HARRIET L. ROBBINS AND C. M. CHILD. least in the ectoderm and body wall, from the head to the level of fission and rises suddenly at the anterior end of the second zooid and of each further zooid, if such are present. It has also been shown that in isolated pieces of the body of Planaria, particularly in those below a certain fraction of the body length, susceptibility is increased during the first few hours following section. This temporary increase of susceptibility, apparently associated with the stimulation of the pieces by section, is least in pieces nearest the head, and increases in suc- cessive pieces posteriorly to the level of fission, where it again decreases (Child, '14). In general the regions of highest sus- ceptibility in the intact animal show the least increase in sus- ceptibility in isolated pieces, and vice versa. Within twelve to twenty-four hours after section the temporary increase in sus- ceptibility has disappeared and the susceptibility of each piece is about the same as, or slightly lower than before section. From twenty-four to forty-eight hours after section a gradual rise in susceptibility begins as the regulatory processes leading to the development of a new individual make themselves evident, and the susceptibility after regeneration is completed is much higher than that of the region of the body of the original animal which a given piece represents. This is true not only for the newly developed head and posterior end of the regenerated animal but for the whole body (Child, '15, Chap. V). In the light of various facts, these changes in susceptibility have been inter- preted as indicating and in some way associated with changes in rate of oxidation. The present paper constitutes additional evidence for this conclusion. Carbon dioxide production was chosen instead of oxygen consumption as the subject of investi- gation first because investigation of the temporary changes, immediately following section involves the technical difficulty of preparing very large numbers of pieces within a very short time in order that oxygen consumption may be sufficient for determination in short periods, and second because work on oxygen consumption during the course of regeneration was already in progress in this laboratory. The colorimetric method was used since it is well adapted for obtaining comparative data with small amounts of material. CO2 IN RELATION TO REGENERATION IN PLANARIA. 1 05 A few experiments similar to those recorded below were per- formed by Child from time to time during several years past, and these experiments, although they brought to light certain difficulties as regards technique and were too few in number to constitute conclusive evidence, indicated very clearly that the differences and changes in susceptibility following section and after regeneration are paralleled by differences and changes in rate of carbon dioxide production. A more extensive investi- gation of the subject by another person was desirable, and this was undertaken by Miss H. L. Robbins in candidacy for the degree of M.S. The data tabulated in the present paper are those obtained in this investigation. The special technique for the different experiments is described in the following sections, but the general technique used in connection with the colorimetric method is very similar to that employed in the study of CO2 production during starvation (Child, '190). The weighing of the animals or pieces is the most difficult feature of the preparation, since the presence of superfluous water introduces an error in weight, and since weighing must be done as rapidly as possible to avoid injury or death from drying. After considerable practice, involving repeated weighings of the same lots and determination of the length of time which could be allowed without injury for drainage on filter paper and exposure to the air while weighing, a satis- factory method of procedure was developed, and this was fol- lowed in all the tabulated experiments. The container was first weighed and both container and weights were left on the balance pans : next weights equal to the estimated weight of the lot of worms or pieces were placed on the weight pan in order to reduce the time necessary for weighing the animals: the worms were then brought together at the tip of a funnel of well washed filter paper, drained for a certain length of time, transferred on a slightly vaselined scalpel blade to the container and weighed, each of these operations being performed in as nearly as possible the same length of time in each case. Equal or approximately equal weights of the lots to be compared were used (see tables). For the colorimetric estimations the Hynson Westcott and Dunning H ion outfit with phenolsulphonephthalein as indicator IO6 HARRIET L. ROBBINS AND C. M. CHILD. was used. After weighing, the worms were returned at once to water and then transferred to pyrex tubes fused at one end and of the same diameter as the standard tubes. After washing twice in aqueous indicator solution of the same concentration as that in the standard tubes, each tube was filled with indicator solution to a 3-c.c. level previously marked. The tubes were then sealed without air bubbles by running in on the surface of the fluid about i c.c. of soft paraffin at a temperature just above melting point, the worms being kept at the bottom of the tube to avoid injury from change of temperature. Since leakage past the paraffin plug is difficult to avoid when changes of tem- perature occur and since it was found desirable to reduce the temperature slightly as a means of keeping the animals quiet (see below), the following method of providing for changes in volume of the fluid was used. A piece of closely fitting soft rubber tubing, previously coated with soft paraffin was drawn over the open end of the pyrex tube, leaving 2 to 3 cm. of the tubing beyond the end of the glass. Indicator solution was then added to fill both the glass tube above the paraffin plug and the rubber tube, and the latter is then closed by a screw clasp. This procedure makes impossible the entrance of air past the paraffin plug when the temperature is lowered. Instead of air a small amount of the indicator solution above the plug may be drawn below, but this occurs within the first five minutes or less of the experiment, and with the changes of temperature involved the amount of fluid passing the plug is negligible, so far as the results are concerned. The indicator solution between the paraffin plug and the rubber tubing is visible and serves as a control for the color change below the plug. Closure by means of the rubber tubing and clamp alone was found to be unsatisfac- tory because some of the worms creep into the rubber tubing, where they cannot be seen, and it is therefore impossible to determine whether motor activity is going on and whether all are in good condition. Moreover, pieces in the rubber tubing are often overlooked when the lots are removed from the exper- imental tube and the whole lot becomes valueless for further experiment, unless substitution for the lost pieces is made, but this is at best an undesirable procedure. CO2 IN RELATION TO REGENERATION IN PLANARIA. IO7 The pH at the beginning of the experiment was of course the same in all lots to be compared, but the starting point differed somewhat in different experiments, the extreme range being 7.8 to 7.95. Observations were made at least every half hour with few exceptions, but the tables, instead of recording all the pH readings, give the times required to reach an arbitrary end point, Hp 7.3 being selected as this end point. As each lot approached this point, observation was more or less continuous. A daylight lamp was used for all color comparisons. A serious difficulty in the earlier experiments, particularly with the pieces after section, was the occurrence of motor activity, which of course increased COs production and introduced a source of error. In these experiments as in earlier work, it was observed that pieces from regions near the head are much more likely to show apparently spontaneous motor activity during the first few hours after section, than pieces from the more pos- terior levels of the first zooid. After attempting in various ways to eliminate motor activity, it was found that decrease of a few degrees in temperature was usually effective for the length of time necessary. In all the experiments tabulated below the animals were kept at 21° to 22° C in the stocks and during prepar- ation, but as soon as the tubes were sealed they were placed in water at a constant temperature of 18° in very dim diffuse daylight. Under these conditions motor activity occurred only rarely, but its occurrence was always noted in the record of the experiment. The procedure adopted in these experiments has been described at some length because in experimental work with animals of such small size it is extremely easy to go astray, if the various sources of error are not carefully controlled as far as possible. THE STIMULATION OF PIECES FOLLOWING SECTION. The temporary increase in susceptibility following section and its characteristic relation, both to size of piece and region of body (Child, 14, also p. 104 above) are so clearly shown by the susceptibility method that they are often used as laboratory experiments. Because they are temporary and -apparently excitatory in character and so definitely related to size of piece 108 HARRIET L. ROBBiNS AND C. M. CHILD. and region of body these changes in susceptibility are of special interest in connection with the question of the relation between susceptibility and metabolism. If changes in the rate of funda- mental metabolism or of certain fundamental reactions are found to parallel these changes in susceptibility, it is evident that the susceptibility method, when properly used, is a rather delicate indicator of at least certain aspects of metabolic condition. In the earlier experiments of Child, as well as in the preliminary work of Robbins, it was found that the changes and differences in CO2 production following section appeared more clearly in animals which had been starved for a few days before experiment, than in those which had been more recently fed, although in the former the total CO2 production and oxygen consumption are less than in the latter (Child, 'iqa, Hyman, 'IQ&). It has been pointed out elsewhere (Child, '196, 'igc and various earlier papers), that the susceptibility method as used in these experiments gives in- formation primarily concerning conditions in ectoderm and body wall, though with certain precautions it may be used to show differences in condition in the alimentary tract of Planaria and other forms. The susceptibility data, as well as other facts, indicate that the changes following section are at least in large measure confined to ectoderm and body wall, the alimentary tract not being affected to any great degree. The CC>2 production of the alimentary tract in fed animals constitutes, however, a large proportion of the total CO2 production, moreover, the volume of the alimentary tract as compared with that of other organs is greater and a larger amount of food and reserves is usually present in regions near the mouth than in regions near the head, therefore it is desirable to decrease this alimentary CO2 production as far as possible, in order that changes in other parts of the body may appear more clearly. It has been shown (Child, 'iga, Hyman, '196) that a rapid decrease in both COa production and oxygen consumption occurs in Planaria doroto- cephala during the first few days of starvation and that an in- crease in both follows so rapidly after even a single feeding that it cannot be due to oxidation of the food following assimilation, but must be due to stimulation of the alimentary tract by the food. Allen ('19) has recently recorded the occurrence of similar CO2 IN RELATION TO REGENERATION IN PLANARIA. 1 09 changes in oxygen consumption in two other species of Planaria during the early stages of starvation. It is evident that the rapid decrease in CC^ production and oxygen consumption during the early stages of starvation is due in large measure, if not wholly, to the decrease in functional activity of the alimentary tract in the absence of food newly ingested. In the light of all these facts the reason for the use in these experiments of animals which have been starved a few days is evident. The length of the period without food is given in each experiment: in no case is it long enough to produce any marked reduction in size or other changes except those in the alimentary tract. Since the purpose of these experiments is to determine whether differences and changes in susceptibility following section are paralleled by differences and changes in CO2 production, the size of animals and pieces used and the regions of body included are those which show the most definite and characteristic differ- ences and changes in susceptibility. The experimental material for the data presented in Table I. was prepared as follows: animals sixteen to eighteen mm. were selected from well fed laboratory stock, those which had recently undergone fission being excluded, were kept without food for several days (see Table I.) and were then cut into pieces as indicated in Fig. I, piece C being cut so that the greater part of the pharynx is separated from its attach- ment and is extracted from the pharyngeal pouch, i.e., piece C contains a part of the pharyngeal pouch, but no portion of the pharynx. Pieces A and C were used in the experiments as repre- senting respectively the most anterior and the most posterior portion of the first or chief member or zooid in animals of this size (Child, 'lib). In the intact animal the susceptibility of the region corresponding to piece A is very much greater than that of the region corresponding to piece C. Immediately after section the susceptibility of piece A shows either a slight increase or no marked change, while the susceptibility of C is increased to such a degree that it is equal to or even greater than that of A (Child, '14). The region B between A and C is intermediate both as regards original susceptibility and the changes following section and is not used in these experiments. The increase in suscepti- bility following section, which is most marked in the C-piece is no HARRIET L. ROBBINS AND C. M. CHILD. A B C temporary and after six to eight hours is in course of disappearance and sooner or later (12 to 24 hours under ordinary conditions) the susceptibility of the piece becomes about the same as or a little less than that of the corresponding regions in the intact animal. These temporary changes in susceptibility indicate that the pieces have been stimulated by section, anterior pieces least, posterior pieces most, and that the condi- tion of excitation disappears after a few hours. It has been suggested elsewhere (Child, '14) that the difference in the degree of stimulation in anterior and posterior pieces results from the greater dependence of the more posterior regions upon impulses from regions anterior to them, so that when the paths of these impulses are cut the condition of the more posterior levels is more affected than that of the anterior levels. This is in agreement with the observations of various investigators concerning the difference in intensity of reaction in many animals between levels anterior and those posterior to a trans- verse cut. The region of the body posterior to C in Fig. I is not used in the tabulated experiments be- cause it consists of one or more zooids indicated by differences in physiological condition (Child, '10, 'nc). Since the number of these posterior zooids differs in different animals and no mor- phological boundaries are visible, pieces from the same levels of this region in different ani- mals are not always strictly comparable physio- logically and therefore merely complicate the experimental data. Each experiment on CO2 production in the pieces A and C consists essentially in determining the rate of change in pH, once as soon as possible after section and again several hours later, of one lot of ^.-pieces and one of C-pieces, consisting of thirty to fifty pieces each and of as nearly as possible equal weight, in equal volumes of indicator solution at a constant temperature FIG. i. CO2 IN RELATION TO REGENERATION IN PLANARIA. Ill of 18° C. As soon as £#7-3 is reached in the first determination, the tubes are opened, the pieces are returned to water and left un- disturbed for the desired number of hours, i.e., at least long enough as determined by the susceptibility method for disappearance in large measure of the temporary changes following section. At the end of this time the rate of pH change is again determined for each lot under as nearly as possible the same experimental conditions as the first determination. Fifteen such experiments were performed before the technique used in the experiments recorded in Table I. was fully worked out. In eight of these fifteen experiments the A -pieces showed marked motor activity and a more rapid change in pH than the C-pieces immediately after section, in three there was either death of some of the pieces from drying or an error in weighing, and in four none of these sources of error was involved and the result were similar to those in Table I. Table I. gives the data for thirteen experiments, in all of which the technique was as nearly as possible the same. The only feature of the table which requires explanation is the column 'Time after section." The times given in this column are the times of sealing of the tubes containing the animals in indicator solution. The first time for each lot, "Immed.," i.e., imme- diately, means simply that the pH determination was begun as soon as possible after the pieces were sectioned. Since it requires a half hour or more to cut lots consisting of forty to fifty A and C-pieces, the time between section and sealing may be as much as an hour for the pieces cut first and only a few minutes for those cut latest. In eleven of the thirteen experiments recorded, 84 per cent., the CO2 production of the C-pieces immediately after section is about equal to, or greater than that of the A -pieces. As regards the other two experiments, Nos. 7 and 8, in No. 7 three C-pieces died during the first determination, and in No. 8, motor activity occurred in the ^.-pieces, perhaps because of a slight rise in temperature of the water in which the tubes were kept. The second pH determination, begun at various lengths of time ranging from nine to forty-two hours after section, shows in every case a lower rate of CC>2 production in C than in A . 112 HARRIET L. ROBBINS AND C. M. CHILD. TABLE I. A COMPARISON OF ANTERIOR AND POSTERIOR PIECES IMMEDIATELY AFTER SECTION AND A NUMBER OF HOURS LATER. Expt. No. No. of Wt. in Pieces. Grams. No. of Days Without Food. Tempera- ture in Degrees C. pH Start- ing Point. Time After Section in Hrs. and Min. Time in Hrs and Min. to Reach pH 7-3- A 37 0.023 II 17-5 7.8 immed. 22:30 5:40 5:40 I C 31 0.0231 II 17-5 7-8 immed. 22:30 5:25 5:55 A 49 0.0255 24 IS 7-9 immed. 9:00 6:25 6:30 2 C45 0.0257 24 18 7-9 immed. 9:00 6:15 6:45 A 56 0.0245 21 18 7.82 immed. 19:40 6:05 3 C 48 0.0243 21 18 7.82 immed. 19:40 5:05 6:23 A 20 0.0182 8 18 7.83 immed. 18:40 6:10 6:22 4 C 23 0.0185 8 18 7.83 immed. 18:40 6:OO 6:45 A 51 0.0208 9 18 7.83 immed. 20:15 5:30 5:35 5 C44 0.0207 9 18 7-83 immed. 20:15 5:05 5:55 A 37 0.0174 12 18 7.83 immed. 18:00 6:30 6:35 C 42 0.0176 12 18 7-83 immed. 1 8:00 6:10 6:45 71 A 37 0.0143 13 IS 7-83 immed. 21:30 7:20 7=45 C 37 0.0142 13 18 7-83 immed. 21:30 7:35 8:00 K~ A 40 0.0216 7 18 7.83 immed. 17:45 4:50 5:05 o C4i 0.0213 7 18 7-83 immed. 17:45 4:55 5:10 A 36 0.0238 8 18 18.5 7.83 immed. 19:00 4:50 438 93 C 36 0.0239 8 18 18.5 7.83 immed. 19:00 4:35 4:50 A 37 0.0242 9 18 7-83 immed. 22:00 4:40 4:55 10 C 38 0.0241 9 18 7-83 immed. 22 :oo 5 • ^ 5 A 44 0.0242 7 18 7-95 immed. 19:00 5:30 5:35 1 1 C45 0.024 7 18 7-95 immed. 19:00 5:10 5:50 A 44 0.0306 8 18 7-95 immed. 42:00 4:45 4:40 12 C 50 0.0305 8 18 7-95 immed. 42:00 4:50 A 29 O.02I 13 18 7-8 immed. 25:00 4:05 4:10 13 C 21 O.0208 13 18 7.8 immed. 3:50 25:00 4:25 CO2 IN RELATION TO REGENERATION IN PLANARIA. 113 As regards the two records for the same lot, the A -pieces show in general a slightly lower CO2 production in the second period than in the first, or in some cases about the same. Experiment 9, however, shows a slightly higher rate in A during the second period, this being due probably to the slightly higher water temperature during the second period. In the C-pieces the rate is distinctly lower, in many cases much lower in the second period than in the first. Differences in time of five minutes between the records of two lots made at the same time mean only that the two were barely distinguishable as different. Differences of this mangitude between the two records of the same lot can mean no more than that the rate is essentially the same in the two periods. Differences of ten minutes or more are however, unquestionably significant and differences of much greater magnitude appear in the table. The table show's then that immediately after section there is in some cases a slight temporary increase in CO2 produc- tion in the ^.-pieces and a very marked increase in the C-pieces, that is to say the differences and changes in CO2 production following section are in general parallel to the differences and changes in susceptibility. The objection may be raised that the differences in CO2 production are less than would be expected from the stimulation by section, but when it is remembered that there is no reason to believe that the alimentary tract shares in this stimulation except locally at the cut surface and that the pieces do not undergo motor activity during the determination this objection has little weight. As a matter of fact the close parallelism between the data on CO2 production and those on susceptibility indicates that even as regards the temporary changes following section of pieces, susceptibility is in some degree 1 During the first determination in No. 7 three C-pieces died. When the determination was repeated the following day, two C-pieces, which had been cut with the others but not used before, were added to lot C and one piece was removed from lot A to make weights as nearly as possible equal. Only two extra worms had been sectioned, therefore three pieces could not be added to take the place of those dead. 2 Slight motor activity occurred in the A-pieces during the first determination. 3 Here the temperature was half a degree higher in the second determination than in the first, and the A -pieces show a slightly increased rate in the second determination. Footnotes for Table I. HARRIET L. ROBBINS AND C. M. CHILD. a measure of physiological and particularly of respiratory con- dition. CARBON DIOXIDE PRODUCTION AFTER REGENERATION. In these experiments stocks of pieces of the size of A, B, C, Fig. I, were cut from animals 16— 18 mm. and allowed to undergo regeneration until the development of the new individuals was essentially complete, usually about two weeks. From these stocks the experimental lots were selected. These consisted only of normal individuals, or in some cases where the number of normal animals in the regenerated stock was not sufficient, mostly of normal with a few teratophthalmic individuals (Child, 'na) added. Since the pieces undergoing regulatory develop- ment cannot feed until they attain an advanced stage, the animals representing the condition before regulation, with which the regenerated individuals are to be compared, are kept without food for the same length of time as the pieces undergoing regener- ation. Usually a stock of intact worms of the same size, 16-18 mm., as those from which the pieces were taken is isolated at TABLE II. A COMPARISON OF SMALL ANIMALS REGENERATED FROM PIECES WITH LARGE ANIMALS THAT HAVE NOT RECENTLY UNDERGONE REGENERATION OR FISSION, OF THE SAME SIZE AS THOSE FROM WHICH THE PIECES WERE TAKEN. BOTH UNFED. DETERMINATIONS OF pH AT + 18° C. Expt. No. No. of Worms. Wt. in Grams. Period of Re- generation or Starvation in Days. pH Starting Point. Time in Hrs. and Min. to Reach pH 7.3. I 3 large 1 6 regen. O.OII4 O.OII3 IO 10 7.89 7.89 10:25 9:00 2 4 large 22 regen. 0.0163 0.016 II II 7.89 7.89 8:55 6:45 3 4 large 27 regen. 0.0174 0.0176 10 IO 7.89 7.89 8:00 6:15 4 4 large 19 regen. 0.0117 0.0115 14 14 7-9 7-9 14:35 12:35 5 5 large 34 regen. 0.0205 0.0204 14 14 7-9 7-9 9:35 6:50 3 large O.OIOI 21 7-8 10:00 6 7 starved 26 regen. 0.099 14 regen. 7-8 7:45 3 large 0.0109 20 7.8 8:30 7 7 starved 19 regen. O.OII 13 regen. 7-8 7:50 8 3 large 28 regen. 0.0166 0.0164 10 10 7.8 7-8 6:30 3:50 CO2 IN RELATION TO REGENERATION IN PLANARIA. 115 the same time the pieces are cut. This stock merely undergoes a slight degree of starvation, while the pieces undergo starvation for the same period and in addition the regulatory changes. This is the procedure in experiments 1-5 and 8 in Table II., but in experiments 6 and 7 the stock from which both pieces and whole animals were obtained was starved seven days before the pieces were cut. Each experiment in Table II., includes one lot of worms about 5 mm. in length which have developed from pieces cut ten to fourteen days earlier ("regen." in table), and one lot of as nearly as possible the same weight of worms 16-18 mm. in length, the same size and from the same general stock as the worms from which the pieces were cut, and kept without food for the same length of time ("large" in table). In all cases the pH deter- minations are made before feeding is resumed. Examination of the last column of the table shows that the rate of CC>2 production is much higher in the small regenerated, than in the large old animals, i.e., the regulatory processes have been accompanied by an increase in rate of CC>2 production. Moreover the rate is in general higher in the regenerated animals TABLE III. A COMPARISON OF SMALL ANIMALS REGENERATED FROM PIECES WITH LARGE ANIMALS OF THE SAME SIZE AS THOSE FROM WHICH THE PIECES WERE TAKEN. Fed three times. Determinations of pH at 18° C. Expt. No. No. of Worms. \Vt. in Grams. pH Starting Point. Time in Hours and Minutes to Reach pH 7.3. j 3 large O.OI27 7.82 5:00 10 regen. O.OI25 7-82 4:30 4 large 0.0193 7.82 3:45 2 15 regen. 0.0191 7.82 2:55 4 large 0.0154 7.82 4:50 3 13 regen. 0.0155 7-82 3:35 4 large 0.0135 7-9 7:15 4 i 8 regen. 0.0134 7-9 5:50 5 large 0.0233 7-9 5:05 5 28 regen. 0.023 7-9 3:45 6 2 large heads off O.OIS5 7.8 8:15 1 8 regen. O.OII3 7.8 4:25 than in the pieces of Table I. although the period without food is in most cases longer in the latter than in the former and CO^ production decreases during the early stages of starvation. Table III. records experiments similar to those of Table II. Il6 HARRIET L. ROBBINS AND C. M. CHILD. except for the fact that both lots were fed with beef liver three times before weighing and pH determination, the first two feed- ings being on successive days, the third after an interval of one day. In the first five experiments of Table III. worms from the first five experiments of Table II. were used, but in smaller numbers because of the increased weights after feeding, partic- ularly in the regenerated animals. In Table III., as in Table II. the "large" animals are those which have not undergone regener- ation and represent as nearly as possible the animals from which the pieces were taken, and the "regenerated" animals are those which have developed from the pieces. Table III. agrees with earlier work (Child, '190) in showing that the rate of change in pH is increased in all animals by feeding after a period of star- vation, but it also shows that the difference in rate between the regenerated and the large animals persists after feeding. Here again the data on CC>2 production agree with the results of the susceptibility method (Child, '15, Chap. IV.). Data on oxygen consumption recently published by Allen ('19) and by Hyman ('196) also agree with these results. ADDITIONAL DATA. Table IV. includes a number of miscellaneous experiments of some interest. In the course of regulatory development an outgrowth of new tissue occurs at anterior and posterior ends of each piece. All the facts indicate that this tissue, which forms the new head and posterior end, is more or less embryonic in character when it arises and possesses, at least at first, a higher rate of metabolism than the remainder of the piece. In order to deter- mine whether the higher rate of CO2 production in regenerated animals is due solely to the more intense activity of this new tissue or whether the rate is also increased in other parts, the new heads and posterior ends were removed from regenerated animals leaving only the so-called old or less extremely altered tissue of the middle regions. Lots of such pieces were then compared with lots of equal weight of freshly cut ^4-pieces (Fig. i) and of animals 16-18 mm. like those from which the pieces were taken, the heads being removed from these large animals in order to C02 IN RELATION TO REGENERATION IN PLANARIA. make them more nearly comparable in condition with the ^.-pieces and the headless regenerated animals. Experiment I of Table IV. includes one lot of each of the three groups and it is seen that the "old" parts of the regenerated TABLE IV. MISCELLANEOUS DATA. Regenerated animals from which anterior and posterior new tissue has been removed, compared with A-pieces and with large headless animals: whole re- generated animals compared with /1-pieces and with growing worms of same size from stock. Fed or unfed. Determinations of pH at 18° C.; pH at beginning of experiment 7.8. Expt.No. No. and Condition of Animals. Wt. in Grams. Nutrition. Time in Hours ar.d Minutes to Reach pH 7.3. 3 large, heads off. 0.093 Starved 19 days. 8:25 I 12 A-pieces. 0.094 Starved 19 days. 7:i5 ii regen. new 0.095 Starved 18 days. 6:25 tissue off. 9 A -pieces. 0.08 Fed three times. 8:15 2 22 regen. new O.O82 Fed three times. 7=30 tissue off. 3 large. O.OIII Fed three times. 7=35 3 15 regen. new 0.0108 Fed three times. 6:05 tissue off. 22 A -pieces. 0.0167 Starved 10 days. 4:10 4 28 regen. 0.0164 Starved 10 days. 3:50 ii small. 0.0096 Fed three times. 6:25 1 8 regen. 0.0095 Fed three times. 5:25 animals show a higher rate of change than the ^4-pieces, while the headless large animals show the lowest rate of all. In Experiment 2 the "old" parts of regenerated animals are compared with A -pieces, both lots being fed three times before sectioning. The result is the same as in experiment I, the rate being distinctly higher in the parts of regenerated animals than in the A -pieces. In experiment 3 the "old" parts of regenerated animals are compared with large old animals from which the heads have not been removed, both lots being fed three times after a star- vation period of about two weeks. The result is the same as in Experiment I, the parts of regenerated animals showing the higher rate. Experiment 4 is a comparison of A -pieces with entire regen crated animals, i.e., including the new heads and posterior Il8 HARRIET L. ROBBINS AND C. M. CHILD. ends. Here the difference in rate is proportionally about the same as in Experiment 2, but somewhat less than in Experiment I . In Experiment 5 regenerated animals are compared with stock animals of slightly larger size (the smallest in the stock at the time) which were kept without food while the pieces were regenerating, both lots being fed three times before the experi- ment, and both consisting of entire animals. Here again the regenerated animals show a higher rate of CO% production than the slightly larger stock animals. A few other experiments performed by one of us and in some cases also by students in the laboratory, are briefly mentioned here without tabulation of the data. It has been found, for example, that the degree of increase in both susceptibility and COz production occurring in regeneration depends upon the degree of reorganization which occurs. Consequently the smaller the piece in relation to the size of the body from which it is taken, the greater the amount of increase in susceptibility and CO%. Similarly in natural fission the posterior piece is not only smaller than the anterior but develops a new head at the anterior end and a prepharyngeal and pharyngeal region by reorganization and redifterentiation within the piece, while the anterior fission piece develops merely a new posterior end. In the animal developed from the posterior piece susceptibility and COz production show a marked increase while in the anterior animal the only marked change in susceptibility is a slight increase in the posterior region, where reorganization and growth have occurred and the increase in CO2 production is either slight or inappreciable. Allen ('19) has recently recorded somewhat similar results as regards oxygen consumption, an increase occurring in the posterior, but not in the anterior product of fission. It has also been determined by one of us that susceptibility to lack of oxygen increases during the development of a new individual from a piece, the susceptibility of the new individual about two weeks after section of the piece, being distinctly higher than that of well fed animals of the same size and about the same as, or slightly than that of animals of the same size, kept without food for the same length of time as the regenerating CO2 IN RELATION TO REGENERATION IN PLANARIA. IIQ pieces. In other words, the new individuals developed from pieces show a susceptibility to lack of oxygen equal to or greater than that of much smaller younger animals than those from which the pieces were taken. These observations concern primarily the susceptibility of ectoderm and body wall. CONCLUSION. These experiments constitute a new test of the validity of the susceptibility method as a rough comparative means of deter- mining physiological or metabolic condition and at every point the differences and changes in susceptibility, as determined by KNC and in many cases by various other agents also, are paral- leled by differences and changes in rate of CC>2 production. Even the temporary stimulation of the pieces after section, which is slight or absent in the A -pieces and very marked in the C-pieces, appears in the data on CO2 production and the increase in rate, at least of respiratory metabolism associated with regulation, is evident in the marked increase in rate of CO2 production even in the "old" parts of the regenerated animal. This work may per- haps be regarded as in some respects the most delicate test of the relation between susceptibility and respiration which has been made up to the present. As has been repeatedly stated, the susceptibility method is not an exact quantitative method, but a rather crude means of indicating differences of some sort in physiological condition and the differential susceptibility of different regions of the same individual affords a means of modifying and controlling various developmental and other processes. The facts at hand concerning susceptibility, e.g., the lack of specificity, the close relation between susceptibility and physiological activity in development, growth and function as well as the positive evidence already obtained concerning the parallelism between suscepti- bility, oxygen consumption and CO2 production indicate very clearly that a more or less definite relation exists between the susceptibility of living protoplasm, to at least many external agents and conditions within certain ranges of concentration or intensity, and the rate or intensity of certain fundamental physiological processes, particularly those which liberate energy. 120 HARRIET L. ROBBINS AND C. M. CHILD. This is all that is meant when susceptibility is interpreted in terms of metabolism or oxidation and the exact nature, degree and extent of this relation of course remains to be determined. This interpretation does not involve the assumption that sus- ceptibility must always be parallel or even proportional to total oxidation or even to total oxygen consumption or COo production. The relation between susceptibility and oxidation is undoubt- edly indirect in at least most cases and it is conceivable that susceptibility may be related only or primarily to certain oxi- dative reactions or to conditions associated with them. More- over, it is certain that in many cases susceptibility as determined by death and disintegration is dependent primarily upon con- ditions or reactions in particular regions of the body, e.g., in ciliate infusoria the ectoplasm, in Planaria the ectoderm and body-wall. Moreover, as many investigators have pointed out, it is by no means certain that oxygen consumption and CO2 production are exact quantitative measures of oxidation at any given time. It is to be expected that susceptibility will not always be proportional to total oxygen consumption or CO2 production, but even then susceptibility may prove in the long run to be a better indicator or comparative measure of physiologi- cal condition than the respiratory data. From what has been said above and in earlier papers (e.g., Child, 'IQC) it is evident that the criticisms of the susceptibility method recently advanced by Lund ('i8a, &) and Allen ('18, '19) need no discussion here, since they are largely beside the point and result from failure to grasp the conception of susceptibility, which has developed from many different lines of investigation, not from one alone. Even if we grant the correctness of certain of their conclusions from experimental data which are or appear at present to be in conflict with conclusions reached in this laboratory (Child, 'iqa, b, c, Hyman, '190, b) on the basis of more extensive investigation, with more satisfactory technique and several different methods instead of one they do not con- stitute adequate grounds for denying the physiological signifi- cance of susceptibility, but rather merely a starting point for the further analysis of the particular cases in question. As regards the real significance of susceptibility, it makes CO2 IN RELATION TO REGENERATION IN PLANARIA. 121 little difference whether or not it shall be found to run exactly parallel to the other indices of total respiration in any particular case. It cannot, however be denied that a wide range of facts determined by many different lines of investigation do indicate clearly the existence of a more or less definite relation between susceptibility and oxidation in at least many cases, and it is of interest to determine range, degree and nature of this relation. The present paper like several others which have recently ap- peared from this laboratory is a contribution to this problem, but it must be remembered that data such as these are not the only criteria of the physiological significance of susceptibility and its relation to the energy-liberating reactions in the metabolic complex. SUMMARY. 1. The colorimetric estimation of CO2 production shows that the changes in CC>2 production following section in pieces of the body of Planaria dorotocephala run parallel with changes in sus- ceptibility. Immediately following section CO2 production is markedly increased in pieces cut from near the mouth region, while in pieces from regions near the head it is only slightly if at all increased. These changes are temporary excitations fol- lowing section and disappear after a number of hours. 2. The development of a new individual from a piece is accom- panied by a very considerable increase in COs production which involves not only the new outgrowths at the two ends of the new animal but the "old" parts as well. This increase in CO2 production is found both before and after feeding is resumed following the development of the piece. In these respects also the changes in CC>2 production parallel changes in susceptibility, both series of data indicating that the animal developing from an isolated piece becomes in the course of this development, physiologically younger than the animal from which the piece originated. REFERENCES. Allen, G. D. "18 Quantitative Studies of the Rate of Respiratory Metabolism in Planaria, I. Amer. Jour. Physiol., XLVIII. '19 Quantitative Studies, etc., II. Amer. Jour. Physiol., XLIX. Child, C. M. '10 Physiological Isolation of Parts and Fission in Planaria. Arch. f. Entwickelungsmech., XXX., II. Teil. 122 HARRIET L. ROBB1NS AND C. M. CHILD. 'ua Experimental Control of Morphogenesis in the Regulation of Planaria, BIOL. BULL., XX. 'ub Studies on the Dynamics of Morphogenesis, etc., I. Jour. Exp. Zool., X. 'uc Studies, III. Jour. Exp. Zool., XL '133 Studies, etc., V. Jour. Exp. Zool., XIV. 'i3b Studies, etc., VI. Arch. f. Entwickelungsmech., XXXVII. '14 Studies, etc., VII. Jour. Exp. Zool., XVI. '15 Senescence and Rejuvenescence. Chicago. 'iga. A Comparative Study of Carbon Dioxde Production During Starvation. in Planaria. Amer. Jour. Physiol., XLVIII. The Effect of Cyanides on Carbon Dioxide Production in Planaria dorotocephala. Amer. Jour. Physiol., XLVII. Susceptibility to Lack of Oxygen During Starvation in Planaria. Amer. Jour. Physiol., XLIX. Hyman, L. H. '193 On the Action of Certain Substances on Oxygen Consumption, II. Amer, Jour. Physiol., XLVIII. 'igb Physiological Studies on Planaria, I. Amer. Jour. Physiol., XLIX. 'IQC Physiological Studies on Planaria, II. Amer. Jour. Physiol., L. Lund, E. J. 'i8a Quantitative Studies on Intracellular Respiration, II. Amer. Jour. Physiol., XLV. 'i8b Quantitative Studies, etc., III. Amer. Jour. Physiol., XLVII. SUSCEPTIBLE AND RESISTANT PHASES OF THE DIVIDING SEA-URCHIN EGG WHEN SUBJECTED TO VARIOUS CONCENTRATIONS OF LIPOID- SOLUBLE SUBSTANCES, ESPECIALLY THE HIGHER ALCOHOLS. FRANCIS MARSH BALDWIN. INTRODUCTION. That dividing Arbacia eggs show periods of varying suscep- tibility and resistance when exposed to chemical substances and to various physical conditions has been proved by numerous investigations. When eggs, from a single fertilized lot were placed at regular successive intervals after fertilization in cyanide- containing sea-water (m/ioo to w/2Oo), Lyon'2 found that they were highly resistant to poisoning fifteen or twenty minutes after fertilization, while eggs exposed to the same solution at the time of cytoplasmic division were promptly killed. Later, after the first division had been completed, the resistance to poisoning again returned, followed by a second susceptible period at the second cleavage. Loeb3 later noted that the unfertilized eggs show greater resistance to cyanide poisoning than the fertilized eggs, and Mathews4 indicated that in dividing eggs, the period of maximum susceptibility is "immediately before and during segmentation," and that just after segmentation the egg becomes relatively highly resistant. Similar results were obtained by Spaulding5 in experiments with weak solutions of ether (1/64 per cent, in sea- water). The period of high resistance continued up to the beginning of the first cleavage, and then fell during cleavage to zero, with a sharp rise immediately afterwards. There was a short period of susceptibility immediately following 1 From the Marine Biological Laboratory, Woods Hole, Mass., and the Depart- ment of Zoology, Iowa State College, Ames, Iowa. 2 E. P. Lyon, Amer. Jour. Physiol., 1902, Vol. 7. p. 56. 3 J. Loeb, Biochem. Zeitschr., 1906, Vol. i, p. 200. 4 A. P. Mathews, BIOL. BULL., 1906, Vol. n, p. 137. 6 E. G. Spaulding, BIOL. BULL., 1904, Vol. 6, p. 224. 123 124 FRANCIS MARSH BALDWIN. fertilization. He found also in acid and salt solutions (pure isotonic KCL and NaCL) a similar but less clearly denned rhythm of susceptibility. Eggs subjected to heat, electrical stimulation and hypertonic sea-water behave in a similar manner. Thus, Lyon,1 observed that the eggs were most resistant to heat at a time previous to the first cleavage, and were most readily injured at the time of division. A. R. Moore2 finds that the resistance to hypertonic sea-water is least "immediately before and during each cytoplasmic division, and that the maximal resistance is shown 35 to 45 minutes after fertilization and just after each division." More recently Lillie3 (1916), has made an extensive study of the rhythmical changes in the resistance of the dividing sea-urchin egg to hypotonic sea-water, and has discussed the physiological significance of this rhythm. His experiments show clearly that at or about the time of formation of the cleavage furrow, a marked decline takes place in the resistance of the egg to hypotony, and cytolysis is then rapid and complete. After the cleavage furrow is fully formed the original resistance returns. A similar reversible decline of resistance takes place at the second and third cleavage, and is probably general for mitotic cell- division. The minimum of resistance is found during the for- mation of the furrow. Both the decline and the return of resis- tance are rapid, the greater part of each phase occupying four to five minutes. Some increase of susceptibility is apparent ten or twelve minutes before the first appearance of the furrow. Similar observations have been made by Herlant4 in the egg of Paracentrotus lividus. From such experiments it appears that the resistance of the eggs to a variety of injurious agencies is least at the time when they are undergoing rapid change of form. To account for these rhythmical changes in the physiological state of the egg, Lillie4 (1909) puts forward the hypothesis that they are essentially the result of variations in the physical condition, especially the permeability, of the surface-film of plasma-membrane, the latter 1 E. P. Lyon, Atner. Jour. Physiol., 1904, Vol. 11, p. 52. 2 A. R. Moore, BIOL. BULL., 1915, Vol. 28, p. 257. 3 R. S. Lillie, Jour. Exper. Zoo/., 1916, Vol. 21, No. 3, p. 401. 4 M. Herlant, Comptes rendus d. I. Societe d. Biologic, 1918, Vol. 81, p. 151. 6 R. S. Lillie, BIOL. BULL., 1909, Vol. 17, p. 207. PHASES OF DIVIDING SEA-URCHIN EGG. 125 undergoing a reversible increase in permeability at the time of cleavage. If a rhythm of alternate increase and decrease of permeability accompanies the rhythm of the mitotic process, it seems logical to infer that the entrance of solutes into the cell would occur most readily when there is a loss of semi-permea- bility. Accompanying this change would be a decrease of the electrical surface-polarization, and this in turn probably would alter the metabolic processes, especially oxidations within the cell. Cell metabolism then is inseparably bound up with cell- permeability; and the plasma-membrane, or semi-permeable surface-layer is something more than a haptogen membrane (to which it has frequently been compared). In discussing this subject in a later paper, Lillie1 makes it especially clear that this ''general characteristic of semi-permeability (the all-essential insulating and diffusing-preventing property) is not merely the result of a special chemical composition and structural density, such as determine the semi-permeability of a precipitation- membrane, but is inseparable from the living condition, i.e., is actively maintained by a continual process of metabolism. The proof of this is that death — the cessation of metabolism — how- ever caused, is invariably followed by a loss of semi-permeability, i.e., the normal state of the membrane then ceases to be main- tained and the unhindered processes of diffusion lead to the disintegration of the cell. Hence destruction of the surface- layer by artificial means — cytolytic substances, heat, extensive mechanical injury — is quickly fatal to all cells." In the experiments about to be described, I have studied the behavior of fertilized Arbacia eggs when subjected for definite brief lengths of time to various concentrations of some of the higher alcohols — anyl, hexyl, heptyl, octyl and capryl — at differ- ent periods of the cell-division cycle. This work was undertaken at the Marine Biological Laboratory, at Woods Hole, Mass., during the past summer at the suggestion of Professor Ralph Lillie, to whom the writer expresses his hearty thanks for many kind suggestions and directions during its prosecution. 1 R. S. Lillie, Amer. Journ. Physiol., 1918, Vol. 45, No. 4, p. 406. 126 FRANCIS MARSH BALDWIN. EXPERIMENTATION. In order to procure a sufficient number of eggs for each series of experiments, between one and two dozen large females were opened, and their eggs collected into finger bowls. By successive washing and settling, a uniform mass of mature eggs was obtained, which could be inseminated and divided into two parts ; one to be used for the control, and the other for the experiments. It was found early in the work that the success of the experiments de- pended upon having batches of eggs which were sufficiently mature and uniform, so that all eggs reached successive stages in their development at practically the same time. It was also found that great exactness in the time-relations of the operations was absolutely essential, and that any variation once entered upon was sufficient to make the results worthless from a com- parative standpoint. Usually two series of experiments were started in a day; one in the morning to be carried over to the gastrula stage by the following morning, and one in the afternoon, to be examined the following afternoon. After extended pre- liminary experimentation, it was found convenient, in any one series, to keep the time of exposure constant and to vary the concentration of substance used, although in a considerable number of experiments the opposite procedure was adopted, i.e., the time was varied and the concentration kept constant. Practically the same procedure was observed throughout the entire experimentation. At each of the successive intervals after fertilization, usually ten minute intervals, about one half of medicine pipette containing a suspension of the inseminated eggs was placed in a small corked Erlenmeyer flask, containing 50 c.c. of the solution of the alcohol in sea-water, and allowed to remain for the time of exposure chosen (usually five minutes). After the given time had nearly elapsed, the excess liquid was poured off, and the eggs with a little of the liquid were placed in a watch glass and the immediate results of the treatment were observed under the low power of the microscope. At the ter- mination of the time of exposure, the watch glass containing the eggs was carefully immersed in a large volume of sea-water in a finger bowl and the water was changed several times to rid it of the excess substance. Finally the eggs were very carefully PHASES OF DIVIDING SEA-URCHIN EGG. 127 washed with a stream of water from the medicine-dropper, and set aside to undergo development. The proportion proceeding with development to the free-swimming larval stage was sub- sequently determined. It was found that the estimate of the proportion surviving to the blastula stage was more readily and exactly made, if the watch glass containing the eggs was removed from the bowl of sea-water just before the free-swimming larval stage was reached. Thus all survivors could be confined within a small volume, and the count or estimate easily made. As a rule, the experiments were carried only up to about the time of second cleavage; since the evidence indicates that the same variation of susceptibility occurs in each cell division cycle; moreover diver- gencies between the different eggs in any lot become more pro- nounced as time elapses, and it is important that all eggs of a lot should be in the same physiological state at the time of treatment. At first several preliminary experiments were necessary in order to determine the most suitable range of concentrations to be used, since the time of exposures determined upon were brief, the longest being ten minutes; in some cases of exposures only three minutes were used. In this connection, the tables given by Lillie1 in his paper on the action of various anaesthetics in suppressing cell-division in sea-urchin eggs, were exceedingly helpful. For i-Amyl2 alcohol, he finds 0.45 to 0.4 vol. per cent, a favorable anaesthetic concentration for eggs subjected for two and one half hours, while 0.5 vol. per cent, and above are somewhat rapidly toxic. For Capryl3 alcohol he finds the anaes- thetizing concentrations to range between 0.012 and 0.02, and notes that even in sub-anaesthetic concentrations this alcohol exhibits a relatively high specific toxicity. With the help of these data, and also Fiihner's4 observations showing that in a series of monohydric aliphatic alcohols each member of the group is from three to four times as effective (for equimolecular con- centrations)5 as its immediate predecessor, it became a compara- 1 R. S. Lillie, Journ. Biolog. Chem., 1914, Vol. 17, No. 2, pp. 129-139. 2 Cf. reference just cited; Table VIII., p. 135. 3 Cf. reference just cited; Table IX., p. 137. 4 H. Fuhner, Arch. f. exp. Path. u. Pharm., 1904, LII., p. 69. 6 Capryl alcohol used in exposures of five minutes seemed not to obey this general rule, since in practically all experiments it was used in concentrations nearly three times its computed strength. (See p. 137.) 128 FRANCIS MARSH BALDWIN. tively easy matter to approximate the most suitable concen- tration of each alcohol after the first had been determined. AMYL ALCOHOL. Summarizing briefly the results of preliminary observations it was found that the most satisfactory concentration of i-Amyl alcohol when used with exposures of three to eight minutes, was between 0.7 and 0.9 vol. per cent. Solutions of this strength are sufficiently toxic to prevent many but not all of the eggs thus treated from developing to a larval stage. Solutions weaker than 0.7 vol. per cent, permit practically all eggs to proceed to i -AMU. ALCOHOL. PLOT OP THE CUHVB OP SUSCEPTIBLE AND BESSHm* PRASTO OF THE MT1STHO SEA-UBCH1H BOC3 WHEtl SUBJECTED TO 0.8 VOL PEH OETfT i-AMYL AHJOWL FOB EfGffr HIHUTB AT SUCESSIVE TEH MMDTB the blastula stage with these exposures, with little appreciable difference. At i.o vol. per cent, not more than 20 per cent, of eggs form free-swimming blastulae even when exposed at the period of highest resistance; (e.g., 30 to 40 minutes after fertili- PHASES OF DIVIDING SEA-URCHIN EGG. 129 zation) and above this concentration the toxicity is such that the decline in survivals is very rapid. In 1.25 vol. per cent, solutions, all eggs are killed in all stages with exposures of nine minutes. Table I. summarizes the results of a typical series of experi- ments with i-amyl alcohol. This particular series of experiments was started in the afternoon of July 16, and the observations noted in the third column were carried over into the morning of TABLE I. I-AMYL ALCOHOL. July 16, 1:45 P.M. The fertilized eggs were placed at the intervals after fertilization noted in column i in 50 cc. of 0.9 vol. per cent i-amyl alcohol. Al exposures except the first (i) (6 minutes) were of eight minutes duration. Intervals After Fertilization. Observed Condition of the Eggs at the Time of Removal from Sol. Proportion Forming Blastulse and Con- dition of Remaining Eggs Next Day. (1) 3- 9 m. (2) 10-18 m. (3) 20-28 m. (4) 30-38 m. (5) 40-48 m. (6) 50-58 m. (7) 60-68 m. (8) 70-78 m. (9) 80-88 m. Fertilization-membranes well formed. No marked cytolytic change noted. No marked change in appearance. Uniform. Membranes markedly swollen in some cases. Slight fading of pigment. A few cells plasmolyzed, other membranes markedly swollen. No marked change. Faint indi- cation of cleavage furrow in a few scattered cells. About 65 per cent, have entered the two-celled stage. Some show shrinkage. About 90 per cent, in two-celled stage. Others intact. Practically all in two-celled stage. A few are starting second cleav- age furrow. About 5 per cent, form free swimming blastulae. Consider- able numbers cytolyzed. About 10 per cent, form blastulae. Not so badly cytolyzed; most cells intact. Nearly 30 per cent, free-swim- ming blastulae. Most eggs intact. Between 30 and 40 per cent, free-swimming blastulae. Most others intact but swollen. Large majority (75-80 per cent.) form swimming blastulae. Relatively few (less than 10 per cent.) form surviving blastulae. Between 30-35 per cent, form blastulae. Few (15-20 per cent.) form blastulae. Others intact. 3-5 per cent, form blastulae. Others intact. the following day. A similar series of experiments performed at about the same time with the same alcohol in somewhat lower concentration (0.8 vol. per cent.), but with slightly longer (lo-minute) exposures yielded substantially the same results. On the following day experiments were carried out on eggs subjected to i.i vol. per cent, solutions with only brief (3-, 4- and 5-minute) exposures with the results noted above. 130 FRANCIS MARSH BALDWIN. In the controls about one half of the eggs were in the two- celled stage at fifty-three minutes after fertilization, and at sixty-five minutes between 85 and 90 per cent, were divided. There is a definite period of well-marked susceptibility imme- diately following fertilization; the susceptibility then gradually and progressively declines up to the end of forty-eight minutes (just before the first cleavage). There then follows a very sus- ceptible period just at the time of cleavage. Later the resistant phase reappears until about the time of second cleavage. If the time intervals are plotted as abscissae, and the percentage of surviving blastulae as ordinates, the relationships may be repre- sented in the curve shown in Fig. I. HEXYL ALCOHOL. In exploring the range of suitable concentrations for hexyl alcohol, the next higher member of the series, assuming that it should be approximately three times as effective as i-amyl alcohol, three preliminary experiments were performed. For these, solu- tions of o.i, 0.25 and 0.30 vol. per cent, were used respectively. The time of exposure was shortened to five minutes, for the reason that it was thought the concentrations were, if anything, a little above the optimum. The results clearly showed that the solutions of o.i vol. per cent, was not sufficiently toxic to demonstrate any variation of susceptibility in the eggs, since at whatever period they were exposed practically all eggs survived to the free-swimming blastula stage. On the other hand, the two higher concentrations proved too toxic, so that practically none of the eggs continued their development after subjection to these solutions at any period. The 0.25 vol. per cent, solution, although it suppressed further development, was not quite intense enough in its action to cause cytolysis in the eggs, with few exceptions. The 0.30 vol. per cent, concentration caused very evident cytolysis, and rupture was almost universal. Accordingly, series of experiments were carried out to test the various concentrations between o.i vol. per cent, and 0.25 vol. per cent. Two of these experiments are summarized in Table II., and may be regarded as typical. These results show a much less definite evidence of a rhythm of PHASES OF DIVIDING SEA-URCHIN EGG. 131 TABLE II. HEXYL ALCOHOL. August n, 10:15 A.M. Fertilized Arbacia eggs were placed at intervals noted in 50 cc. of 0.13 and 0.17 vol. per cent, of Hexyl alcohol respectively, and allowed to remain in them for five minutes. They were then placed in watch glasses quickly observed, and treated as described in the previous experiment. Intervals After Ferti- lization. (A) 0.13 Vol. Per Cent. (B) 0.17 Vol. Per Cent. Observed Condition. Proportion Forming Blastulae. Observed Condition. Proportion Forming Blastulse. (i)i5-20m. Fert. membrane Majority (90 per No marked cyto- Between 70 and well formed. No cent.) form lysis. Uniform 80 per cent, form marked cytoly- blastulae. batch of fertile blastulae. Other sis noted. eggs. cytolyzed but intact. (2) 25-3om. No marked Between 85—90 Fert. membrane 70-75 per cent. change. per cent, form in most eggs blastulae. Few swimming blas- swollen; no ruptured. tulae great change otherwise. (3)3S-4om. Membrane swol- Practically all No marked 90 per cent, form len, few show (90 per cent, or change noted. blastulae. slight plas- over) form blas- molysis. tulae. Others intact. (4) 45-Som. Slight loss in igo per cent, form Slight fading of Between 65-70 pigmentation. blastulae. Few pigment. No per cent, form No marked cy- ruptured. other marked blastulae. Few tolysis however. change. scattered cells ruptured. (5)55-6om. About half in 80-85 per cent. All intact, 50 per Nearly 60 per two-celled form blastulae. • cent, or over in cent, form bias- stage. No Others mostly two-celled tulae. marked change. intact. stage. (6) 65-7om. Fully 85 percent. About 76 per No change. Not more than 50 in two-celled cent, form per cent, form stage ; no swimming blas- blastulae. Others marked change tulee. badly cytolyzed from preceding. but mostly in- tact. (7)75-8om. No marked Between 65—70 Slight loss of About 60 per change. per cent, swim- pigment. No cent, blastulae. ming blastulse; marked change. Two-celled eggs others badly conspicuous. cytolyzed. Most others intact. (8)85-9om. Few cells (ca. About 60 per Few cells in Nearly 85 per 5-7 per cent.) cent. form second cleavage. cent, swimming show second swimming blas- No marked cy- blastulae. cleavage. No tulae. Most tolysis. marked change, others intact. susceptibility than those just described for i-amyl alcohol. The eggs apparently maintain throughout the cycle a relatively high resistance to the concentrations of hexyl alcohol here used, with only a slight increase of susceptibility at the time of 132 FRANCIS MARSH BALDWIN. first cleavage; in the one case (0.17 vol. per cent.) there is evidence of a slight return of resistance just afterwards, and in the other (0.13 vol. per cent.) there is not. Why there should be this difference in the behavior of the two alcohols is difficult to explain. The exposures to hexyl alcohol were perhaps insufficiently PER CEHT OF HEFTYL ALCOHOL. PLOT OF THE CUHVE OT SUSCEPTIBLE SND RESISTANT FHA3E3 OT THE 3EA-ORCHIH COOS WMJ1 SUBJECTED TO 0.07VOL.FER CENT nEFTYL ALCOHOL FOR 1TVE MINUTES AT TO1 MJMUTE INTERVALS BEOINWHG THIRTY MIHOTE3 AFTER rERTTLIZATlOM. SURVIVORS. THE PER CEHT OF SORYJVIflG BLASTULAC IS PLOTTED XG/UN3T THE TIME IMTESVAL3. USING ran convEnrm >-< C CJ OJ CO -u .S "° CU ' — ' c o r--. 13 I B ft HI ^ >> O (LI O I— I O W < M ^ •< H H S ffi o I M.s .S E _o > "o , ^ S. O ^_; 10 o B > — oo to bo B II 2 5 S -o y -M -0 a'E PH «»a n •< •S S JJS • § g "8 • -S J2*J> ft cs ^ S „ . .^4J 3cx2c 1 ill C/3 - ' ii CO +j +J ii •— TO Cu lli G CO -+-» III ba E E E E U-) uo l^ IO ro ^t" LO O i i . 3 ^ "5 c . "o o. . 5 3 01 , i ™" ?Ti ^ ~*~* a ^ 2 X o3 O* m ° Hi ,0 2 d ~~rt •c •£ r^e .Q Cu 2 ""* & <§' 3 ° £%6 d ^ *^ 2 a o •3 o ^ £ "aj ~ S ^ ° >> "S -a ^ •3 ^ QJ 6/) flj ^^ C CJ *^^ CO ^ CH o U o .2 d ai -^ •u £*•> ^» .1 •*-* S 4_J ? • •y M " a 1 ~n "*-* i^ *^ I 'I ^ '^ O U .^H CO ^-t-H E !l x1 o ^ e »1 ^> 0) OJ >> tn d N > ^ d i.^-S-2 5 i- °w .a .13 OJ O ^J 4) M ^ CJ d 0 a. £ N 3 §j • ' ^ £ ^ g^^> a. -t-J (L* ^3 Cd rf q ,| 0 0 *, ^ CJ O -4-t -4-> r^ pq o £ S O 2 "O aJ M « ;| "^ -y o C a • "§) c JS o "tn '"* "*^ O •a flj C/J fli u .1 •o u il ? ,2r rt CX^ ^ E O U <-i-i 0^0 o o\ ^ >H > 0. 3 ^ T^ /"^ JH >-i o ^ •*-* o oj ^ aJ O ? 3 ^ cJ ^3 VO (U CJ <+H ^ J-, -J_> 4) +J X! C D q pq fe d S c jl"g EC o ai o 3 O •3 SJ S ^ c o c ^ ^ 0) m o U •O U >>T3 "o *J D *j C > >> 2 > ri o £.fi fe - " 2 1 O^ aJ u O S ^ .*- CO S S "5 10 ir> J>* oo fc i A c ». — f 03 136 FRANCIS MARSH BALDWIN. as typical the data of the 0.07 vol. per cent, solution, an interest- ing curve is obtained (Fig. 2) which is fairly comparable with the one shown for i-amyl alcohol. There is a gradual rise in resistance up to the period of first cleavage, with a sharp drop during cell-division followed by a slow recoverry. OCTYL ALCOHOL. Normal octyl alcohol is apparently considerably more toxic than its isomere capryl alcohol. In a series of five experiments with octyl alcohol in concentrations ranging from o.oio to 0.030 vol. per cent., the best concentration for five minute times of exposure was found to be in the neighborhood of 0.015 VOL Per cent. On the other hand the outcome of fourteen experiments with capryl alcohol showed the optimum concentration for the same time of exposure to be between 0.035 and 0.045 v°l- Per cent., which is between two and three times the favorable con- centration of normal octyl alcohol. Table IV. summaries a TABLE IV. NORMAL OCTYL ALCOHOL. Fertilized eggs were subjected for five minutes to 0.013 vol. per cent, of normal octyl alcohol at intervals of ten minutes. Intervals After Fertilization. Observed Condition on Removal from Fluid. Observed Condition the Following Day. (1) 15-20 m. (2) 25-30 m. (3) 35-40 m. (4) 45-50 m. (5) 55-60 m. (6) 65-70 m. (7) 75-8o m. (8) 85-90 m. Fertilization membrane well formed. Slight loss of pigment. No noticeable cytolysis although very marked loss of pigment. Decided loss of pigment. No marked change in membrane or cytoplasm. About 2 per cent, show first furrow. Slight loss pigment. No cytolysis. Over half in first cleavage. About 90 per cent, in two-celled stage. Aside from loss of pigment no noticeable change. A few (i per cent.) just begin to show second cleavage furrow No marked change in appear- ance. Nearly 50 per cent, active blas- tulse. Others badly cytolyzed, few ruptured. About 65 per cent, active blas- tulse. Others cytolyzed. Nearly 80 per cent, active. Others intact. Practically all active blastulae. About 85 per cent, active. Almost 60 per cent, active blas- tulte, numbers of two-celled egg present and mostly intact. Some badly cytolyzed and ruptured. Between 65 and 70 per cent, active blastulae. Most others cytolyzed but intact. Between 85 and 90 per cent, active blastulse. Others cy- tolyzed but intact. PHASES OF DIVIDING SEA-URCHIN EGG. 137 typical experiment using normal octyl alcohol of 0.013 vol. per cent, concentration. For exposures of five minutes duration, this concentration gave the best results, and showed very clearly the resistant and susceptible phases. CAPRYL ALCOHOL. As mentioned before, experiments with various concentrations of capryl alcohol showed that for brief exposures, the most favorable concentration was nearly three times that of normal octyl alcohol. This may perhaps be accounted for in some measure by the fact that not all samples of capryl alcohol are ree wpRn. ALCOHOL. CEffT TLOT OF CDHVES HHH1 US1HO 0 035 and 0.045 TOL. FER CEHT OF CJ1PRT1. ALCOHOL FOR FIVE MIHUTB EXPOSURES AT SUCCESSIVE TEN SOSVIVOBS. HHHTEE IHTEHFAlS. 10 83 TIME. uniform in chemical composition and purity; a slight difference in this respect is known to make a decided difference in its chemical and physiological activity. In suitable concentrations, this alcohol is without doubt one of the most satisfactory for 138 FRANCIS MARSH BALDWIN. showing susceptible and resistant phases in dividing eggs. Several experiments were tried with this alcohol in which the concentration was kept constant and the time of exposure was varied; and from the data thus gathered, it seems probable that of the two factors, concentration is the more important. In other words, if the concentration is such that it gives the best TABLE V. Fertilized eggs were subjected for five minutes at ten minute intervals to 0.035 and 0.045 v°l- Per cent, capryl alcohol. Intervals. (A) 0.035 Vol. Per Cent. (B) 0.045 Vol. Per Cent. Observed Condition. Condition Follow- ing Day. Observed Condition. Condition Follow- ing Day. (l) IO-I5IT1. Membranes well 75-80 per cent. No marked dif- About 65 per formed. No active. Others ference from cent active. great difference cytolyzed but normal eggs. Others badly from normal mostly intact. cytolyzed. eggs. (2)20-2501. Membrane About 83 per No marked Nearly 85 per slightly swollen. cent, active. change. cent, active. No marked Others badly Others intact. cytolysis. cytolyzed. (3)30-35m. Few show loss About 92 per Marked fading Between 85-90 of pigment, cy- cent, active. of pigment. No per cent, active. toplasm shrunk- Others intact. marked cytoly- Some ruptured en in some cases. sis. eggs motile. (4) 40-45m. No marked About 95 per No marked iAbout 70 per change. None cent, active change, slight cent active. show cleavage blastulae. loss of pigment. Others mostly furrow. Others intact. intact. (5)50-55m. Nearly half show 80 per cent, ac- No marked About 50 per first cleavage tive blastulae. change. cent . active furrow No Few ruptured. blastulae. marked change. Most others badly cytolyzed, many ruptured. (6) 60-6501. Nearly all show About 50 per Practically all in About 40 per first cleavage cent, active two cell stage. cent, active. All furrow. No blastulae. Some loss pig- others badly marked cytoly- Most others ment. ruptured. Some sis. ruptured. still in two cells. (7)?o-75m. No marked About 30 per No marked Nearly 30 per change. Few cent, active change, except cent, active. scattered cells blastulae. loss of pigment. Others badly show second Others badly cytolyzed. Few furrow. cytolyzed. persist in two cells. (8)80-85111. No marked Nearly 80 per No marked Nearly 50 per cytolysis. cent, active change. cent . active blastulae. blastulae. Others mostly Others cytoly- intact. zed but mostly intact. When plotted the data gives interesting curves as shown in Fig. 3. PHASES OF DIVIDING SEA-URCHIN EGG. 139 results with a five-minute exposure, when the exposure is pro- longed to eight minutes, very little or no difference is detected. This generalization, however, could probably be applied only within narrow limits. The data from two experiments using capryl alcohol in 0.035 and 0.045 v°l- Per cent, concentrations respectively are given in Table V. These records are fairly typical of results of other experiments. SUMMARY. 1. The developing sea-urchin egg when subjected to suitable concentrations of various lipoid-soluble substances — i-amyl, hexyl, heptyl, octyl and capryl alcohols — shows unmistakable rhythms of susceptible and resistant phases, which when taken in connection with the earlier observations of Lyon, Herlant, Mathews, Spaulding, Lillie and others, constitute additional evidence that a very intimate relation exists between the general physiological condition of the egg, and the physical state of its plasma-membrane. 2. During the first ten or fifteen minutes after fertilization the eggs are more susceptible than at any other time until the period just preceding division. A comparatively resistant phase gradually becomes more and more marked up to just before the first cell-division (about 45 or 48 minutes after fertilization). This is followed by a period of decidedly increased susceptibility which lasts for about 15 or 20 minutes, during which time marked! cytological effects are noted. Subsequently the resistant phase is largely recovered, and maintained up to the time of the second cleavage. 3. The most favorable concentrations of the various alcohols for demonstrating the rhythm of susceptibility range as follows: i-amyl, between 0.7 and 0.9 vol. per cent.; hexyl, between 0.13 and 0.17 vol. per cent.; heptyl, between 0.06 and 0.07 vol. per cent.; normal octyl, about 0.015; while capryl was considerably above its isomere (normal octyl) between 0.035 and 0.045 vol. per cent. The best records were obtained in experiments using i-amyl and capryl alcohols, possibly indicating a higher specific toxicity of these when compared to the others. 4. When suitable concentrations were used, no marked 140 FRANCIS MARSH BALDWIN. differences could be detected by varying slightly the durations ot exposure. Eggs exposed for five, eight or even ten minutes to the same concentration gave similar results. This, however, would probably apply only within narrow limits. LIGHT PRODUCTION IN CEPHALOPODS, I. AN INTRODUCTORY SURVEY. S. STILLMAN BERRY, REDLANDS, CALIFORNIA. CONTENTS. PAGE 1. Introduction 141 2. Classification of Cephalopods 144 Tetrabranchiata 144 Dibranchiata 144 Octopoda 145 Decapoda 145 Synoptic Tables 146 3. Distribution of the Photogenic Function Among Cephalopods 147 Preliminary Remarks 147 Synopsis of the Genera and Species 149 Summary of Recent Cephalopoda 157 Summary of Preceding Tables 158 Recapitulation of Data 158 4. Actual Observance of the Phenomenon 159 i. INTRODUCTION. Recent interest in the subject of biophotogenesis has been so great, and bids so fair to continue at high ebb, until at least the problem of the economical artificial production of chemical light has been solved, that for the use of the many classes of investi- gators, most of whom are not zoologists and can scarcely be expected to possess accurate taxonomic knowledge of the group with which they may chance to wish to work, it would be exceed- ingly desirable if there could be placed on record in compact form a summary of all the species of each principal division of plants and animals which are known or thought to possess photogenic properties. The writer's desire to see this service performed on behalf of the Cephalopods, animals which must always stand well up with the highest in the estimation of the student of or- ganic light, furnished the initial stimulus which has finally broadened into the production of the present paper. As a taxonomist, however, and, in so far as this particular group of 141 142 S. STILLMAN BERRY. animals is concerned, one of that despised species, the "closet naturalist" —he can only go a certain way with his subject, and by the same token, his remarks must perforce have only a very limited value. Yet the effort seems worth while spending, and he can fairly plead in extenuation of his temerity, if not of his own limitations, that cephalopods are such active, delicately balanced creatures, and so exquisitely adjusted to an environment in which it is next to impossible to observe them accurately, and which it is even more vain to attempt to establish, even partially, under artificial conditions, that the difficulties of subjecting the details of their life history and ecology to that searching exami- nation required by the standards of modern biological investi- gation have proven practically insurmountable. Therefore the unfortunate circumstance that we have no specialist in this branch, — no authoritative student of the bionomics of cephal- opods, and that such halting summarization as can be done must be handled by the systematist or general student of the group, if at all. Admitting then the largely pragmatic and temporary rather than permanently intrinsic value of the present dissertation we may proceed with it, for even so small a contribution as this can pretend to be should prove helpful. Amid the wealth of remarkable features, structural and physi- ological, with which the entire group of the Cephalopoda entices the student, the variety and multiplication of those which in an earlier day would have been as unquestioningly as delightedly hailed as adaptive are supremely conspicuous. These are special for the most part to the conditions and vicissitudes brought about by an exceptionally active manner of life in an environ- ment full of actual or potential diversity. Not even the fishes are better swimmers, nor, with all their aristocratic vertebrate organization, lead a more complicated struggle for existence. It is perhaps concomitantly both a result and cause of all this that the group so fairly teems with bizarre cells, tissues, organs, complexes of organs, which, as we may as well admit without further parley, can scarcely be interpreted otherwise than as marvelously exquisite adaptations, each to its own definite end. Such knowledge of most of these as we possess has been amassed almost wholly since the time of Darwin, else the pages of the LIGHT PRODUCTION IN CEPHALOPODS. 143 "Origin" might have been enriched by many examples as start- ling in their way as any of the classical ones. The complex cephalopod chromatophore, the inter-playing system of exactly balanced musculature with scarcely any hard skeletal parts to give it support and leverage which goes to make up the arms and each single sucker, the delicate adjustment between eye, sucker and chromatophore through the mediation of the nervous system to result in one of our most perfect demonstrations of concealing coloration, the innumerable types of hectocotylus often involving the most astonishing modifications in sexual behavior, the amazing and still insufficiently understood mech- anism of the spermatophore, the eyes, and, without attempting to prolong the list further, the photogenic organs, — each is in its own way a triumph of adaptive development, how much so we may perhaps infer to some extent from the widespread occurrence of these structures in one form or another among nearly all the now-surviving cephalopods. Continuing with the structures last named, for instance, I think it can be truly said that no other class of animals can compare with the cephalopods in the complexity, diversity, beauty, brilliancy, — in brief, the high specialization of organs devoted to the production and utili- zation of that form of energy which to our human faculties finds expression as light. It has been said with considerable show of truth that the generation of light by the plasm of animals and plants is really far less to be marveled at than the transformation of their energy into motion. But motion is practically a general prop- erty of protoplasm in all its forms, without which it could scarcely exist as living substance at all. The reason why the production of organic light appears so remarkable to most of us is more special: it is partly of course because of the apparent economy of this light so far as the dissipation of heat energy is concerned, but mainly to the average observer because of its evident highly specialized adaptation to certain particular ends. Dubois wrote in I8Q5:1 'The most resplendent of all animals are insects, of which class the glowworm, beloved of the poets, is one of the most brilliant examples." Cephalopods were scarcely Smithsonian Report, 1895, p. 418. 144 s- STILLMAN BERRY. noticed as being luminous at that period, but now we know that the firefly and glow-worm pale in comparison, and that probably not even the brilliant display of the tropical elaters can vie with the gorgeous pyrotechnics of certain squids. It is indeed quite possible that the latter exhibit the highest development of the photogenic function known in the entire animal kingdom. 2. CLASSIFICATION OF CEPHALOPODS. Living members of the Molluscan Class Cephalopoda cleave simply and naturally into two well-defined, easily separable groups. The first of these, and that universally regarded as the most primitive, is the Order Tetrabranchiata, comprising only the few species contained in the single genus Nautilus. The animals of this group are characterized especially by the possession of a massive, chambered, external shell; of a "funnel" formed by the appression of two lateral folds which remain unfused in the median line below; of a system of suckerless lobes around the mouth, bearing retractile, annulated tentacles; of such traces of metamerism as the presence of two pairs each of ctenidia, "branchial hearts," auricles, renal organs, and osphradia; and of a simple "pin-hole" eye, open to the exterior. Ink-sac and chromatophores are absent. The second group, Order Dibranchiata, comprising all living cephalopods except Nautilus, is characterized by either the complete atrophy of the shell or its reduction in the adult to a concealed loose coil (Spirulidae), a calcareous plate (Sepiidse), or a horny pen; by an entire, tubular funnel; by the development of the anterior portion of the primitive foot into a series of eight or ten muscular, sucker-bearing, tentacle-like arms about the mouth; by but a single pair each of ctenidia, branchial hearts, auricles and renal organs; by the presence, at least typically, of highly developed eyes; by the development of complex, special- ized pigment cells in the skin; and by the presence of the peculiar "ink-sac." Osphradia are absent. The Dibranchiata in their turn are sharply divisible into two subgroups, the Decapoda and Octopoda. The former are mainly pelagic; have finned, generally elongate bodies; have not only the eight "primary" arms of the octopods, but a pair of LIGHT PRODUCTION IN CEPHALOPODS. 145 specially modified "tentacles" as well; have the suckers pedun- culate, reinforced with a chitinous ring, and often very curiously modified; and have a so-called buccal membrane surrounding the mouth. The Octopoda are principally shore or bottom-loving forms; short bodied; lack fins, or have them only secondarily developed; have eight arms only, with their suckers sessile and lacking chitinous rings; and lack the buccal membrane. There are also important internal characters which need not concern us here, but it may be said that few living groups are more sharply de- limited. As a whole the group of the Decapoda has seemed to most students more archaic than the Octopoda, at any rate is less uniformly divergent from what must have been the ancestral stock, but it includes many highly specialized types, and prob- ably neither group as we now know it can be taken a ^ S s 1-4 a, O P 0 H § U z u D O § z 3 D O a u u Q Pterygio- rcuthis giardi. 1 1 1 1 1 1 II 1 1 HH M hH HH O IN W ^ H ^s Pterygio- teuthis gemmata. 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 II 1 1 i „ 33 i 3 i CS (N ' *t ' rt. ro 111 1 1 1 1 1 1 "5 II 1 1 i 3 3333 i O tN (N CS ^" ' M ?§ 11 "at as en ia scintillans 3 3 1 i i ++++++ 2 O II 1 1 1 1 1 8 ^_ + 3 0 ^ M Aln-aliopsis sp. 3 Siii ++++++ 2 o II 1 1 1 1 1 8 + 3 •0 h-l |-tp 3 3 1 i ++++++ 2 '§ 11 1 1 1 1 ~- 8 + 3 M Lamfiadio- teuthis iiifgaleia. i i i i i i S 11 •O N 1 1 N 00 'rT ^T ^M" ^g ~ <•> r^. "TO M "~ ^<: 0) M M l-t f^ M + u |l§ 1 1 1 1 1 X 1 ' 'V-^ O hH 01 01 1 o -^- (N N PI CS , •§. ^ •*- o o o ^ ii 'O pO 41 t >-.!-, fl"-i323t,3 0; OC 'c S K * K ^ o •• ^* *o *o rn "T3 ^3rtnJOJCrt^o3 OO -^^^"ti^'tifj^-^c^^MW MoggSS.SoSia ».&.& Ba»»u^> j SHH « C/3 < Ventro-laterals . Tips of ventrals ; ; ! o : - • • ''•''• 1 en OJ w •-« *p fj . Ql W |^J »™^ .^5 . *^ •• ^C ^C o_> ^C CQ CJ ^C w o o E^ ii p Total number Probable nurn LIGHT PRODUCTION IN CEPHALOPODS. 189 The photogenic systems of all the species of the eight genera having polymorphic organs are outlined in further detail in Table VII. Those species considered having the mere largest number of photophores are the three Abralioids, occupying the three central columns of the table, but those exhibiting the highest degree of polymorphism are Lampadioteuthis megaleia, which has not been investigated histologically but must have not less than seven or eight types of photophores in all, Lycoteuthis diadema, with ten types, or thirteen, if the accessory organs are counted in, and Nematolampas regalis. Lycoteuthis diadema, with the immeasurable advantage of having had its marvelous photogenic properties observed in the living state, is usually cited as the example par excellence of a luminous cephalopod. However, it is evident from sheer morphological grounds that even this wonderful creature must yield the palm to another, if nearly related, genus and species, — the truly amazing Nemato- lampas regalis of the Kermadec Islands. Whether this species will ultimately be found to display all the varied brilliance of the red, white, and blue lights of Lycoteuthis, the fact remains that in addition to a complete series of exactly homologous organs, it has an entire battery of pyrotechnic engines of its own, so there is every reason to expect a more rather than a less elaborate illumination. The total number of photophores in this species is in excess of ninety, which are elaborated upon no less than twelve or thirteen different structural principles of uncertain homologies with one another. Counting in the three types of accessory photophores which are to be found in the eight "double" organs (proximal tentacular, terminal subocular, and anal), the total number of types is increased to fifteen or sixteen. Which of the alternative figures quoted is the correct one is still to be established by histological work. 9. SYSTEMATIC SIGNIFICANCE OF PHOTOGENIC ORGANS. It follows almost as a corollary from what has been said in the foregoing sections of this paper that the photogenic system evinces a complex of features of the utmost value to the taxono- mist. Of late years ever increasing weight has been given to it, and the presence of constant differences, even though minute, in I9O S. STILLMAN BERRY. its components, is now admitted practically without debate as ample ground for taxonomic discrimination. Where such differ- ences are shown to occur, further differences in the remaining organization seem practically predestined for eventual discovery. Good characters for specific discrimination are to be found, not only in the presence or absence of photogenic organs, but also in their distribution on or within the body, in their number, in their size, and in the veriest details of their intrinsic structure. The taxonomist has in fact few more convenient points of attack in the pursuit of his primary objects of classification and relation- ship than that afforded by the light organs. And this is exactly what we find, if to somewhat less degree, among the fishes and the few other groups where the photogenic organs have attained some considerable complexity. One can construct a fairly workable taxonomic key based on the photogenic organs alone, for such species as possess them. 10. PROBABLE POLYPHYLETIC ORIGIN OF PHOTOGENIC ORGANS. Before concluding this paper a somewhat general answer may be attempted to a question which has no doubt occurred more than once in the mind of the reader, and which indeed has been touched upon very nearly on more than one occasion — Is photo- genesis a primitive function among cephalopods? In other words, are our present day species descended from an ancestral photogenic stem, some branches of which have now yielded up the function? Or has photogenesis arisen several times in this class of animals, possibly to meet altogether diverse conditions or associations in the environment, so that its presence therefore becomes of secondary ratl er than primary significance? At first glance the widespread distribution of the function in the great and, comparatively, primitive oegopsid group of cephalo- pods favors an affirmative answer to our first query. But in reply to this it may be said that the varied pelagic environment of these forms would almost per se favor the development of the light-producing function after a manner which would be hardly likely to hold true among the more littoral Myopsida and Octo- poda, the former of which are mainly frequenters of much shallower water than the (Egopsida, the latter hardly ever LIGHT PRODUCTION IN CEPHALOPODS. pelagic at all, and then generally surface forms or confined to the shallower water like so many of the myopsids. There are many other arguments which may militate against any theory of monophyletism and as strongly support the con- trary view as brought out by the last query above. These, having already been largely elaborated elsewhere or to be dealt with in another connection later on, need be merely summarized here. Such considerations are: 1. The uneven distribution of photogenic organs throughout the entire group, and, as a corollary of this, their appearance in distantly related groups more or less sporadically. 2. The variety and sporadic character of the development of photogenic organs in different regions of the body. 3. The large number of strongly diverse structural types. 4. The evidence from ecological considerations, the distribution upon the body, and similar facts that these organs have arisen in response to very diverse environmental requirements. How then may one bespeak a photogenic sy 'stem? Exactly as one speaks of a muscular system, or a skeletal system, or a receptor system in almost any animal body. The term is used in the sense not necessarily indicating an aggregation of homo- logous structures, but an assemblage of organs within a single organic body exhibiting more o'r less similar or coordinate physiological reactions, if at times neither in fact phylogenetically nor ontogenetically related. ii. CONCLUDING NOTE. This paper is mainly a compilation from the scattered work of other authors. No doubt there are omissions, but the aim has been to present simply a concise summary of the knowledge of this subject which has been gained to the present time. It cannot be too strongly emphasized that not only are many more species of luminous cephalopods likely to be discovered in the future, but some of those now known but not yet recognized as possessing photogenic properties are likely to be revealed as having them. Of the known luminous forms some will no doubt prove to possess luminous organs or properties additional to those de- scribed. Bearing all this in mind, if this little paper but fur- 192 S. STILLMAN BERRY. nishes some delving student just a little better base of attack on his problem than might otherwise have been afforded him, its purpose will have been fulfilled. 12. SUMMARY. 1. Light production is an unusually widespread phenomenon in the molluscan Class Cephalopoda. 2. Although unknown in the Order Tetrabranchiata, scarcely developed in the octopod section of the Dibranchiata, and occur- ring little more than sporadically among the Myopsida, over one half of all described (Egopsida are known to possess photogenic properties. 3. The actual production of light by living cephalopods has been observed only rarely, but in species of sufficiently diverse relationship to confirm the evidence drawn from the morphology and histology of organs found in the remaining species. 4. The light of some species exhibits remarkable brilliance. 5. The color of the light emanating from the respective organs within the same species or in different species may exhibit striking differences in both intensity and quality, but it is not known to what extent this is actually due to inherent diversity in the physi- cal properties of the light rays themselves. 6. Photogenic organs may occur in almost any portion of the body in this group of animals, but the outer integument, eyeball and pallial chamber are the situations most favored. They are often internal and able to function only by reason of the trans- parency of the body tissues in the living state. 7. The organs are predominantly, but by no means exclusively, ventral in distribution. 8. The organs are strongly polymorphic, even in the same species, varying from comparatively simple bodies of photogenic tissue to the highly complex "searchlight" types. 9. Numerous duplex organs, or organs with accessory photo- phores, are known to occur. 10. Luminous organs in the Myopsida are usually of the type known as discharging. Those of the other groups are entirely of the enclosed or ductless type. 11. The maximum polymorphism in the photophores of any LIGHT PRODUCTION IN CEPHALOPODS. single species occurs in Nematolampas regalis Berry, from the Kermadec Islands, where the 90 or more organs are elaborated upon 12 or 13 more or less diverse structural principles. 12. The occurrence, distribution, arrangement, and morpho- logical detail of photogenic organs in cephalopods are features of considerable taxonomic importance and yield valuable clues as to the relationship and classification of the genera and species even where still unknown anatomically. 13. The best evidence seems to indicate that the photogenic organs in this group of animals are polyphyletic and more or less sporadic in origin, hence that light production in cephalopods is not an essentially primitive or ancestral function to be regarded as now lost in many members of the group. 13. LITERATURE.16 Berry, S. S. '13 Nematolampas, a Remarkable New Cephalopod from the South Pacific. BIOLOGICAL BULLETIN, v. 25, pp. 208-212, i fig., AuguSt, 1913. 'isa Teuthological Miscellany No. i. Zoologischer Anzeiger, Bd. 42, pp. 590- 592, October, 1913. Chun, C. 'oo Aus den Tiefen des Weltmeeres. Jena, 1900. '03 Aus den Tiefen des Weltmeeres. Zweite Auflage, Jena, 1903. '033 Uber Leuchtorgane und Augen von Tiefsee-Cephalopoden. Verhandl. d. deutschen Zoologischen Gesellschaft, 1903, pp. 67-91, text figs. 1-14, 1903. '10 Die Cephalopoden. i. Teil: Oegopsida. Wissenschaftliche Ergebnisse d. deutschen Tiefsee-Expedition Valdivia, Bd. 18, pp. 1-402, 2 pis. and 32 figs, in text, Atlas of 61 pis., 1910. '13 Cephalopoda. Report Scientific Results " Michael Sars " North Deep Sea Expedition, 1910, V. 3, pt. i, Zoology, pp. 1-28, text figs, i-n, pi. 1-2, 1013. Dahlgren, U. '16 The Production of Light by Animals. Journal Franklin Institute, Feb.- April, 1916, pp. 1-75, text figs. 1-20. Darwin, C. '60 Journal of Researches into the Natural History and Geology of the Coun- tries Visited during the Voyage round the World of H. M. S. " Beagle " under the Command of Captain Fitz Roy, R.N. 8vo, pp. 1-551, illus- trated, London, 1913 (first edition May, 1860). Holder, C. F. '87 Living Lights. A Popular Account of Phosphorescent Animals and Vegetables. 8vo, 127 p., 28 pis., New York, 1887. 16 A more exhaustive bibliography of the subject is in preparation for a later paper of this series. 194 s- STILLMAN BERRY. Hoyle, W. E. '02 The Luminous Organs of Pterygioteuthis margaritifera, a Mediterranean Cephalopod. Memoirs and Proceedings Manchester Literary and Philo- sophical Society, V. 46, No. 16, pp. 1-14, text fig. i— 6, June, 1902. '04 Reports on the dredging operations off the west coast of Central Americ- ... by the ..." Albatross," etc. Reports on the Cephalopoda. Bula letin Museum Comparative Zoology, V. 43, pp. 51-64, pi. 9-10, March, 1904. '08 Address to the Zoloogical Section, British Association for the Advancement of Science, Leicester, 1907. Report British Association Advancement Science 1907, pp. 520-539 [1-20], text fig. A-D, 1908. '09 The Luminous Organs of Some Cephalopoda from the Pacific Ocean. Proceedings 7th International Zoological Congress, Boston Meeting (ad- vance print), pp. 1-5, Cambridge, 1909. Ishikawa, C. "13 Einige Bemerkungen iiber den leuchtenden Tintenfisch, Watasea nov. gen. (Abraliopsis der Autoren) scintillans Berry, aus Japan. Zoologischer Anzeiger, Bd. 43, pp. 162-172, text fig. 1-6, December, 1913. Joubin, L. '93 Recherches sur 1'appareil lumineux d'un cephalopode, Histioteiithis ruppellii, Verany. Bulletin Societe Scientifique Medicale de 1'Ouest, V. 2, pp. 49-80 [1-32], text fig. i-ro, 1893. '933 Note complementaire sur 1'appareil lumineux d'un cephalopode: Histio- teuthis Ruppellii, Verany. Bulletin Societe Scientifique Medicale de 1'Ouest, V. 2, pp. 161-169 [1-9]. 1893. *93b Note sur une adaptation particuliere de certains chromatophores chez un cephalopode (1'oeil thermoscopique de Chiroteuthis-Bomplandi Verany?). Bulletin Societe Zoologique France, V. 18, pp. 146-151 [1-6], i text fig., 1893. '93C Quelques organes colores de la peau chez deux cephalopodes du genre Chiroteuthis. Memoires Societe Zoologique France, V. 6, pp. 331-343 [1-13], text fig. 1-12, 1893. '94 Nouvelles recherches sur 1'appareil lumineux des cephalopodes du genre Histioteuthis. Bulletin Societe Scientifique Medicale de 1'Ouest, V. 3, pp. [1-15], text fig. 1-7, 1894. '95 Note sur les appareils photogenes cutanes de deux cephalopodes: Histiopsis atlantica Hoyle et Abralia oweni (Verany) Hoyle. Memoires Societe Zoologique France, V. 8, pp. 212-228 [1-17], text fig. i-n, 1895. '05 Note sur les organes lumineux de deux cephalopodes. Bulletin Societe Zoologique France, V. 30, pp. 64-69, text fig. 1-2, 1905. '053 Note sur les organes photogenes de 1'oeil de Leachia cyclura. Bulletin Musee Oceanographique Monaco, No. 33, pp. 1-13, text fig. 1-7, April, 1905. '12 Etudes preliminaires sur les cephalopodes recuellis au cours des croisieres de S. A. S. le Prince de Monaco, ire Note: Melanoteuthis lucens nov. gen. et sp. Bulletin Institut Oceanographique Monaco, No. 220, pp. 1-14, text fig. 1-12, January, 1912. 'iza Sur les cephalopodes captures en 1911 par S. A. S. le Prince de Monaco. Comptes Rendus Seances Academic Science, Paris, V. 154, pp. 395-397 [1-3], 1912. Meyer, W. T. '06 Uber das Leuchtorgan der Sepiolini. Zoologischer Anzeiger, Bd. 30, pp. 388-392, text fig. 1-3, July, 1906. LIGHT PRODUCTION IN CEPHALOPODS. 1 95 '08 Uber das Leuchtorgan der Sepiolini: II., Das Leuchtorgan von Hetero- teuthis. Zoologischer Anzeiger, Bd. 32, pp. 505-508, text fig. 1-4. January, 1908. Naef, A. '12 Teuthologische Notizen, 1-2. Zoologischer Anzeiger, Bd. 39, pp. 241-248, March, 1912. Sasaki, M. '12 Hotaru-ika. Toyama-ken, Sui-san Kumiai Hokoku, pp. 1-29, 2 pis. and chart, 1912. (In Japanese.) '13 Hotaru-ika. Zoological Magazine, Tokyo, V. 25, pp. 581-590, pi., 1913- (In Japanese.) '14 Observations on Hotaru-ika Watasenia scintillans. Journal College Agri- culture, Tohoku Imperial University, V. 6, pp. 75-107, i text pi., pi. 1-3, November, 1913. Tryon, G. W., Jr. '79 Cephalopoda. Manual Conchology, V, i, pp. 1-316, pi. 1-112, Philadel- phia, 1879. Verany, J. B. '51 Mollusques mediterraneans, observes, decrits, figures et chromolitho- graphies d'apres le vivant, I., Cephalopodes de la Mediterranee. G£nes, 1851. * Vivanti, A. '14 Contributo alia conoscenza dei Cefalopodi abissali del Mediterraneo. Ricerche sulla Carybditeuthis maculata n. g. n. sp. dello Stretto di Messina. Archivio Zoologico Italiano, V. 7, pp. 55~79. text fig. i, pi. 3~5. I9M- Watase, S. '05 Luminous Organs of Abraliopsis, a New Phosphorescent Cephalopod from the Japan Sea. Zoological Magazine, Tokyo, V. 17, PP- 119-122. l text fig., June, 1905. (In Japanese.) RAPIDITY OF ACTIVATION IN THE FERTILIZATION OF NEREIS. H. B. GOODRICH. The following experiments were designed as a test of the rapidity of the action of the spermatozoon of Nereis in relation to the initiation of the processes of maturation. The almost instantaneous effect of the contact of the spermatozoon in stimu- lating the formation of the fertilization membrane and, in this case, of the jelly is well known. It is also usually observed that, in the case of those eggs in which maturation follows insemination, that polar-body formation will occur without fail even if later observation shows that no cleavage follows. It might be con- ceived that the continued action of the spermatozoon were necessary to cause maturation. The results outlined below indicate that maturation in Nereis follows almost, if not quite as brief an application of the stimulus as is necessary to initiate the jelly formation. The experiments of Lillie ('n) on Nereis limbata showed clearly that the removal of the spermatozoon later than twenty- one minutes after insemination by a process of centrifuging did not interfere with the maturation of the egg. It was, how- ever, found impossible by this method to remove the sperm earlier than twenty -one minutes after fertilization. Various workers have suggested (I am indebted to Dr. Chambers for first calling this to my attention) that the Barbour apparatus for micro-dissection offered a means of removal during this earlier period. The manipulation of the instrument for this p'urpose has proved most successful. The spermatozoon may readily be removed shortly after attachment and with more difficulty later because of the increasing strength of adherence to the egg and the viscidity of the head of the spermatozoon. The viscidity is shown in attempts to remove the spermatozoon at about 35 minutes after insemination. The head, remaining attached in the region of the perforatorium may be extended by 196 FERTILIZATION OF NEREIS. 1 97 the needle some five or ten times its original length. In these later stages the method of removal by centrifuging is more practical. The experiments were made during the summer of 1919 at the Marine Biological Laboratory at Woods Hole, Mass. Sperm and eggs were obtained from animals caught the evening before and kept in the upper compartment of the refrigerator during the night. Sperm dilutions of about 1/400 to 1/500 were used for most of the experiments. This dilution is not such as will insure that only one spermatozoon will come in contact with each egg (Lillie, '15), but it is sufficiently dilute so that poly- spermy rarely results. Polyspermy was noted in a few of the eggs in experiments 6 and 7. The eggs were placed in a dilute suspension of chinese ink, at once fertilized and transferred to the coverslip which forms the roof of the moist chamber. The coverslips had been previously carefully cleaned of all traces of oil or grease in order to allow the drop of water to spread, thus forming a thin film which compresses the eggs slightly as is neces- sary in order to hold the egg firmly during the operation. The apparatus was always in complete readiness for the operation and it was usually possible to locate promptly an attached spermatozoon and to carefully push or rub it from its point of attachment to the egg. Spermatozoa remained immobile after detachment. Two or three operations were usually carried out on each coverslip. More were not attempted because the delay would allow too great an evaporation of water in the necessarily imperfect moist chamber, thus causing greater compression of the egg and also greater concentration of the sea water. The posi- tion of the operated eggs among others was plotted, the coverslip removed and placed under a binocular microscope, the selected eggs were isolated and removed to separate dishes for observation. Frequently the remainder of the eggs on the cover slip was also removed for cleavage counts (Control 2). These were the last to be removed and in some cases had by that time undergone considerable compression (in other cases the cover slip was slightly flooded to prevent this). This may in part account for the low percentage cleavage recorded frequently under Control 2. The eggs were then kept under observation to note the forma- 198 H. B. GOODRICH. tion of the polar bodies and in the second series (Table II.) to note possible cleavage. In all but two of the sixty eggs the polar bodies formed. I am inclined to think that as these two were among the earlier ones observed in this case the negative record may be due to faulty observation. Also in only three cases and one of these from a clearly polyspermic lot of eggs was cleavage observed. The time of removal varied from i% minutes to 13^ minutes after insemination. It is impracticable to remove the sperm in most cases earlier than two minutes after insemination as it is difficult to discriminate between spermatozoa lying against the egg and those that have effected an attachment. The average of elapsed time from insemination to removal was 6.2 minutes and in sixteen cases the spermatozoon was removed in less than 4 minutes after attachment, and in five cases in less than 3 minutes. It seems therefore clear that in so far as maturation is concerned, the full stimulating effect of the sperma- tozoon is effective within a very few minutes after attachment and quite possibly it is only a matter of seconds. The results clearly support the concept that the first phase of activation of the egg-membrane formation and maturation is initiated by the spermatozoon which "activates a substance, or ferment-like bodies, contained within the egg" (Lillie, '19, p. 159), rather than by the continuous introduction of some lysin- like substance through the slender perforatorium. For it seems improbable that the spermatozoon could introduce in less than two minutes through the perforatorium which has a cross section area of perhaps one ten-millionth of the surface area of the egg, a sufficient amount of material to take part in reactions through- out the egg. It should, however, be noted that under certain conditions (heating, Just, '15) it is possible to initiate jelly forma- tion without maturation following. This may be taken to indi- cate that a lesser stimulus gives a lesser result and possibly if it were practicable to remove the sperm more promptly that jelly formation only would result. On the other hand in the cited case we may be dealing with a stimulus differing in kind rather than in degree. It should also be noted that these results are not at variance with the concept that cortical changes form an all important intermediate step in the activation of the egg. FERTILIZATION OF NEREIS. 199 In some cases (Table II.) it was possible to note the time of formation of the first polar body. The average of elapsed time after insemination was 35.8 minutes and no correlation was noted between the duration of attachment of the sperm and the time of formation of the polar bodies. TABLE I. Date. Experi- ment Num- ber. Time of Fertiliza- tion. Egg- Time of Removal of Sperm. Minutes Elapsed. Polar Body Forma- tion. Cleav- age. Controls. No. i. No. 2. July 17 I 10.48 ' A 10.56 8 + — + 60% B 11.02 14 + — + 1 1 2 2.44 A 2-54 10 + _l + 90% B 2-58 14 + _i + " 18 3 5-29 A 5-32^ 31A — _i + 65% B 5-37^ 8^ + _i + ' ' iQ 4 9-59 A 10.03 4 — _i + 70% B io.o8H 91A + i + i i C 10. IO ii + _i + i i 20 5 2.29^ A 2.33J4 4 + — + 60% 6 2.40 B 2.51 ii + — 2 + " 21 7 9-40lA A 9.48 7^ + +2 + 90% 8 io.i4lA A 10. 18^ 4 + + + 40% B 10.21% 7M + + + ** 9 11.26 A n.27% i% + 1 + 50% B 11.32 6 + 1 + tt 10 2-54lA A 3-00 51A + — + 70% B 3-03 &yz + — + " ii 3-13 A 3-I6H 31A + _1 + B 3-21 Yi 81A + _1 + As noted in Table I. some eggs were fixed at about time of the first cleavage with view to study of cytological changes. Too few of these have survived the ordeal of embedding and sectioning to make any detailed study valuable. I noted that in some cases only chromosomal vesicles and in other cases well-formed chro- mosomes were present at the time of the first cleavage spindle. This may be compared with the observation of Lillie ('15) where chromosomes were formed in eggs from which sperm were re- moved by centrifuging but not in eggs which were caused to maturate by centrifuging without insemination. The appended tables outline the experiments. Full data are 1 Egg fixed at about time of expected first cleavage. No indication of cleavage at that time. 2 Poly-spermy observed in same lot on cover-slip. 2OO H. B. GOODRICH. not present in all cases. The experiments were in part pre- liminary and the desirability of various controls and observation became apparent as they progressed. In as much as the work TABLE II. u rt Q ll ll w •ojd •5 *•• tj! be em W « ° S Jg£ Minutes Elapsed. Polar Body Formation. Time Formed. Minutes Elapsed. Cleavage. Controls. No. i. No. 2. No. 3- July 23 12 9-47 A 9-50 3 + IO.I9 33 — + 60% B 9-5334 634 3 + C 9-5434 734 + 10.20 34 — + " 13 IO.2O A 10.2334 3M + IO.52 32 — + 80% B 10.2534 sH + — + " 14 11.0514 A 11.0834 3 + — -f- 15 H.3634 A 11-3834 2 + — + B 11.4234 6 + — -|- C n-4334 7 + — -{- 16 2.40 A 2.4834 834 + 3-21 31 — + 90% B 2-5334 1334 + 3.17 37 — + ' ' 17 3.1634 A 3.2434 8 no. obs. 4 + B 3-2434 9 + 3-49H 33 4 -f C 3-26M 10 + _4 + D 3-28 n34 + _4 + 24 18 IO.I5 A 10.1834 334 + 10.46 31 4 -j- B 10.22 7 + 10.49 34 4 + 19 I0.5I34 A 10-55 334 + 11.2234 31 4 -f B 10.5734 6 + 4 + C 10.5834 7 no obs. — 4 + 20 11.26 A 11.29 3 + H.5634 3034 4 + B H-3334 634 + 11-5734 3i34 — 4 + 21 3-i8}4 A 3-21% 334 + 3-53 35 _4 + B 3-24 534 + 3-53 35 — 4 + C 3-25 634 + 3-52 34 4 + 25 22 9-33 A 9.36 3 + 10.0534 3234 — -j- 60% B 9.38 5 + 10.06 33 — + ' ' C 9-39 6 + 10. 06 33 — + 1 1 23 10.16 A 10.1834 23^ + — + 58% B 10.2134 534 + — + *' 24 10.4834 A 10.5134 3 + — + 57% B 10.52 334 -(- — -(- * ' C 10.55 634 + — + 11 D 10.57 834 + — + ' ' 25 11-34 A n-3734 334 + 12.15 41 — + 45% B n-4334 934 + — + ' * 26 2.19^4 A 2.4234 4M -(- 2-57 3734 — -j- 25% 21 2.47 A 2-49 2 + 3-25 38 — + 55'- + 28 3.5434 A 3-57 234 + 3-37 4234 — + 80% + B 4.00 534 + 3.36 4i34 — + 80% + 26 29 9-52 A 9-55 3 + 10.27 35 — + ! 66% + 30 10.29 A 10.32% 3/4 + ii. 02 33 — + ' ' + B 10.35 6 + 11.03 34 — + 57% 31 II-0534 A 11.0734 234 + H.3834 33 — + 75% + B II. IO^ 5', -f 11.42 y<> 37 — + * * + 3 Egg disintegrated. 4 No specific record in original notes of the absence of cleavage. FERTILIZATION OF NEREIS. 2OI was stopped by the end of the Nereis " Run" and as the observa- tions in regard to maturation were complete it seems to me to be best to present the work in its present form. TABLES. The headings are mostly self-explanatory. Under Polar Body Formation and Cleavage a plus sign ( + ) indicates that the process was observed and a minus ( — ) the reverse. Control No. i are eggs from the same lots as in the experiments fertilized a few minutes before the experiment began from the sperm of the same male used. Control 2 are eggs not operated on from the coverslip and Control 3 are eggs of the same inseminated lot mixed with chinese ink from which those placed on the cover slip were taken. The plus sign ( + ) under Controls I and 3 indicates the practically 100 per cent, cleavage usually realized with Nereis eggs. WESLEYAN UNIVERSITY, January, 1920. LITERATURE CITED. Just, E. E. '15 Initiation of Development in Nereis. BIOL. BULL., Vol. 28. Lillie, F. R. 'n Studies of Fertilization in Nereis, I and II. Jour. Morph., Vol. 22. '15 Studies of Fertilization, VII. BIOL. BULL., Vol. 28. 'ig Problems of Fertilization. University of Chicago Science Series. IMMUNITY AND THE POWER OF DIGESTION.1 VERA DANCHAKOFF, COLUMBIA UNIVERSITY. The proteins are the fundamental chemical constituents of the living protoplasm in all our tissues and organs. But the cells of the tissues do not take up the proteins as such, they are known to live on the products of protein digestion, on their building stones, the various amino-acids. And by a long process of synthetic character the cells of a multicellular organism build up out of these building stones their own individual proteins of a highly specific structure. The multicellular organism possesses special organs of digestion which successfully perform the work of splitting the ingested food to the stage of amino-acids, in which stage they become capable of passing through the intestinal wall ; they are absorbed here and transported to all our tissues, which in this manner are provided with various substances for their synthetic work in order to replace the wear and tear of the living substance during adult life or to build up organs and tissues during developmental stages. Now are the cells of all our organs and tissues which, under usual conditions, are supplied for their synthetic work with ready made building stones, without one of the most characteristic and important properties of the living matter, viz., the power of splitting the chemically more complex proteins to the stage of amino-acids? Or are these cells not manifesting a digestive potency inherent in them, because they have no chance of manifesting it, because the ingested food does not reach them primarily and undergoes the hydrolytic changes before it is capable of being absorbed? Is it a lack of power or a lack of opportunity which prevents the cells of our tissues from mani- festing a digestive capacity under usual conditions? 1 Given at the meeting of the Section of Biology, New York Academy of Sciences, October 13. 1919. 202 IMMUNITY AND THE POWER OF DIGESTION. 2O3 A most wonderful property exhibited by the tissues themselves is to inhibit growth of foreign tissue. This resistance of the organism against proliferation of any heterogeneous cellular elements, be it normal tissues, tumor cells or microorganisms, is more generally known under the term of immunity. There is little need to emphasize the importance of any new information concerning this problem. Disregarding, however, the most earn- est efforts and attempts to throw upon this property light suffi- cient to master it, no definitive knowledge has yet been acquired concerning the nature of this property. In all organisms, plants and animals as well, the power of resistance against proliferation of any foreign tissue does not seem to depend upon the activity of a special organ in the organism. A graft of heterogeneous tissue simply does not take, a tumor graft does not grow, microorganisms do not pro- liferate, die and disappear in an immune or immunized animal. A living cell endowed with the faculty of proliferation, for ex- ample, a tumor cell, well known for its high rate of proliferation and on account of this property disastrous to the organism, on which it has settled, if grafted on an immune organism, will certainly find here all of the elements needed for the building up of its own protoplasm, for we know that the tissues of the organism are abundantly provided with various amino-acids, and these not being specific, can be used equally well by any tissue. And still a tumor cell will not live on an immune organ- ism. It dies and disappears. If now we inquire into the relation between the digestive power of the cell and resistance in a unicellular organism, we will see that both phenomena in this case are very closely connected. A unicellular organism can ingest another living organism, bac- teria as well and the latter, while within a cytoplasmic vacuole of the former, are killed and consecutively digested by the enzymes of the phagocyte. The unicellular organism in this case proves to be immune against a possible intracytoplasmic proliferation of the ingested living individual, primarily and solely because of its digestive capacity. The question whether in a multicellular organism any relation exists between its resistance against proliferation of a foreign tissue in it and a digestive power of its tissues is obscured by the 2O4 VERA DANCHAKOFF. fact that the tissues of the organism do not exercise under normal conditions any digestive power, and it is not definitely settled, which of the various tissues, if any, besides those connected with the digestive tract, are endowed with a digestive power. And still there exist sufficient indications that the tissues of a multi- cellular organism, besides those highly specialized of the digestive tract, are equipped with enzymes which confer on them a diges- tive power similar to that so characteristic of the special elements in the digestive tract. If the tissues of a multicellular organism be put under the stress of starvation, under which condition no amino-acids will reach them from the absorbing surface of the digestive tract, the tissues will in this case use up tissue proteins, they themselves hydrolyze the proteins or digest them, which work is made possible by the presence of enzymes in these cells. And another example of digestive work performed by tissue cells of a multicellular organism can be found in the growth of tissue on artificial media. Here again the transplanted bits of tissue do not find within the culture medium ready building stones for the building up of their protoplasm in the form of amino-acids, but only proteins of the blood plasma clot. Here again the cells of the tissues themselves have a chance of performing the splitting or digestion of the proteins, and they do it. In both these cases we base our conclusions of the presence of enzymes in the tissue cells on the results of the experiments: on the continuation of the output of nitrogen by the organism during a diet free from nitrogen in the starvation experiment, and on the further growth of the tissue in the culture experiment. The fact only not the mechanism of the digestive activity by tissue cells is determined in both cases. Some information concerning the mechanism, or rather con- cerning one of the mechanisms, by which cells of our body may digest, i.e., split and assimilate protein may be gained by micro- scopical study. And this study informs us that in embryos developing from mesoblastic eggs, i.e., from eggs containing great quantities of yolk, as for example, from birds eggs, all of the tissues contain at an early stage a great quantity of yolk granules in their cytoplasm, which are digested intracellularly and assimilated. Endodermal cells as well as ectoderm and mesoderm cells, even primitive germ cells, all are endowed with IMMUNITY AND THE POWER OF DIGESTION. 2O5 an intracellular digestive capacity. And this capacity is surely not lost during the development of the embryo, at least the mesenchymal cells continue to exercise this faculty, and wher- ever cells in the developing organism have lost their normal correlation with other tissues, as for example, red blood corpuscles during the rearrangement of vessels, mesenchymal cells are seen to ingest them and digest them within their cytoplasm. The mechanism of this digestive activity is analogous to that observed in unicellular organisms. Enzymes are not given off as in the digestive tract, they work within the cytoplasm, they are endo- enzymes. To a digestive activity of the mesenchymal cells is due the disappearance of aseptic emboli, trombi and infarcts; small sequesters may be entirely resorbed and catgut sutures disappear in the organism in a short time. An intracellular digestive activity is exhibited in an intense way by the tissues of embryos developing from mesoblastic eggs in the early stages of embryonic development and only occasion- ally during adult condition. This power is dormant in the tissues of mammals during embryonic development, the embryo receiving all the materials needed for its development in the form of amino-acids from the maternal placenta, but is awakened at the first opportunity of being confronted with unsplit protein. The digestive power of the tissues is well evident in respect to cellular protein in the dead form. Only in a unicellular organism did we see that their digestive activity confers on them a power over living organisms and cells as well and therefore becomes the source and the cause of their immunity against a possible proliferation of the ingested organisms. May the digestive capacity of the tissues of a multicellular organism, of which we have seen a few examples, be also in some way con- nected with the failure of a heterogeneous graft to take? Is this power not the deleterious factor which cannot be overcome by the grafted heterogeneous tissue and which inhibits its growth and proliferation? The results of a long series of experiments, which I am going to illustrate, show without any doubt that at least in some cases it is the digestive activity of an adult mesenchymal cell which inhibits the growth of a heterogeneous tumor or rather destroys the actively growing tumor. The experiments were made with 206 VERA DANCHAKOFF. different tumors, but only the Ehrlich sarcoma, and the tumor known at the Crocker Fund Laboratory, under the number 180, gave demonstrable results. It is known that mammalian tumors may be grown easily on chick embryos, but not on adult animals. Also normal adult chick tissue grows well on embryos of the same species. Therefore, embryonic tissues, more particularly the chick allantois, have been used by me as a culture medium, to bring together mammalian tumors and various adult chick tissues. It was expected that the study of the interaction of the tissues of the adult naturally immune fowl and of the mammalian tumor cells grown in a culture medium, for both equally favor- able, might show whether the digestive capacity of the tissues more particularly that of the adult splenic mesenchymal cells may in any way be connected with the resistance offered by the adult animal to the grafting of the tumor on it. The experiments have shown that the Ehrlich sarcoma gives invariably a good growth if grafted alone, but if grafted in a mixture with the spleen, disappears even after a short period of proliferation. The short-lived fame of the small lymphocytes thought to be re- sponsible for this disappearance is still in our memory. Only an absolute disregard of microscopical findings can explain how microscopical pictures similar to thor.e shown here (Figs. I, 2, 3 and 4), have been overlooked and how the small lymphocyte could become the fetish of the immunity. Microscopical preparations, as seen in retouched photographs, which accompany this paper, illustrate in a striking way the process of disappearance of tumor foci surrounded by the adult splenic mesenchyme in the allantois. Two lines of activity are observed in the mesenchyme, it splits off numerous mobile cells of hemoblastic nature which differentiate further into granular leucocytes. This developmental potency is exhibited in an even more intensive way by the splenic adult mesenchyme, if the splenic tissue is grafted alone on the chick allantois. But the fact of grafting the splenic tissue together with tumor reveals in it a new potency and this is its power of isolating and surround- ing tumor cells, of enclosing them in vacuoles and of digesting them within these vacuoles (Figs. I, 2, 3 and 4). The adult splenic mesenchymal cell is apparently attracted toward the mammalian tumor cell. Contrary to the embryonic mesen- IMMUNITY AND THE POWER OF DIGESTION. 2O7 chyme, the adult mesenchymal cells, once close to the tumor cell, will not indifferently pass by, but together with other mesenchymal cells, will tightly surround it. The tumor cell, in response to the approach of the adult mesenchymal cell, with- draws its cytoplasmic processes, becomes immobile and assumes soon a spherical shape. The Figure I illustrates a tumor focus in which the cells still intensely proliferate in the center as may be seen from numerous mitoses present. The tumor focus is however surrounded by a zone of mesenchymal syncytium of splenic origin. The cells of this tissue encircle the tumor cells at first surrounding them very tightly (Figs. I and 2, x). The adult splenic mesenchymal cells of the fowl treat a heterogeneous mammalian tumor cell, which, if present alone in the embryonic allantois, would live and proliferate in no other manner than if it were a block of dead protein. They gather around the tumor cells and enclose them into a capsule and then secrete a fluid within the little cavity occupied by the tumor cell. The nature of this fluid is such that a disintegration of the tumor cell takes place and a complete splitting of its proteins, it must therefore contain proteolytic enzymes. The tumor cell gradually loses its structure (TV, Figs. 1,2,3 and 4) is transformed into a block of structureless protein (TV, Figs, i, 2, 3 and 4) and finally disap- pears completely. Tumor cell after tumor cell is digested in this manner. In the case of the Ehrlich sarcoma, the rate of digestion of the tumor cells by the splenic mesenchyme is higher than the rate of proliferation of the tumor cells, and the grafted tumor, even after a good start of development, disappears. The same process is observed in relation to the sarcoma 180. Only in this case the rate of proliferation of these tumor cells is higher than the rate of their digestion by the splenic mesenchyme, and the tumor still grows in spite of the existence of a peripheral zone around it, in which the process of digestion of tumor cells by mesenchymal cells is most evident. The study of microscopical preparations allows us to follow the gradual changes which the tumor cells undergo within the vacuoles surrounded by mesenchymal cells, changes which lead to the full disappearance of the tumor cells. Figures 2, 3 and 4 show very clearly how healthy tumor cells H Tc are cut off from the tumor focus, the tumor cells withdrawing their processes 208 VERA DANCHAKOFF. and rounding up, how the tumor cells gradually lose their structure TV, their cytoplasm becoming vacuolized, their nuclei pycnotic and how finally the tumor cells disappear entirely. Now in respect to the small lymphocytes, may they not be still connected in some indirect way with the disappearance of the tumor cells? Round-cell infiltration has been described around the disappearing tumor grafts within the allantois. But though the small lymphocyte is a round cell, not every round cell is a small lymphocyte. Round cell infiltration exists indeed around such grafts. This infiltration, however, at a closer study proves to consist not of small lymphocytes, but of round, mobile cells of hemoblastic nature which, as mentioned above, differ- entiate into granular leucocytes. As has been shown in one of my previous papers, the small lymphocytes are not very viable in the allantois and in most cases quickly disappear in the grafted splenic tissue. It is a positive tropism between the adult mesenchymal cell and the tumor cell expressed in the phagocytic power of the mesenchymal cell over the tumor cell and the digestive activity of the adult mesenchymal cell which prove to be the direct factors in the disappearance of the tumor cells in a double mixed graft of tumor and spleen. One of the remarkable results of the various experiments undertaken in this direction is the fact, that only the adult splenic mesenchyme, not the embryonic, exhibits the capacity of digesting tumor cells. This observation well corresponds with the fact that the embryo fails to resist growth of heterogeneous tissue. I have already mentioned that only in respect to the Ehrlich sarcoma and to the sarcoma 180 could I obtain results which clearly demonstrate the phagocytic and digestive activity of the adult splenic mesenchymal cells. The study of the growth of other tumors and of the check of their growth is sufficiently advanced to enable me to conclude that the inhibition of the car- cinoma growth is effected by a mechanism not altogether iden- tical to that described in relation to the Ehrlich sarcoma cells. Must this fact astonish us? I do not think so. To expect a liter- ally identical response of the mesenchymal cells to different agents would be just as inconsistent as to expect the digestive activity in all multicellular animals to proceed in a perfectly identical IMMUNITY AND THE POWER OF DIGESTION. 2OQ manner. And we know that this is not the case. We know that in multicellular organisms there exists a digestive cavity, into which free enzymes are poured, we know, however, that in sea- anemones, though a digestive cavity does exist, no ferments had ever been discovered in it and the digestion of the food proceeds through the immediate apposition to it of definitecellular elements. The fact that at least in some cases the phagocy tic and digestive activity of the mesenchymal adult cells is found to be the dele- terious factor, which cannot be overcome by some of the tumors and which inhibits the growth of these tumors, or rather destroys an actively growing tumor, throws a new light on the immunity problem in general, since the power of resistance against pro- liferation of foreign tissue, though manifested in different ways, can certainly not be of a fundamentally different nature. 210 VERA DANCHAKOFF EXPLANATION OF FIGURES. The figures are retouched photographs from original preparations. The Figure i at about 400, the Figure 3, 700 and the Figures 2 and 4, 1000 diameters. ABBREVIATIONS. Gr Lc. Granular leucocytes. H Tc. Healthy tumor cells. 5 Ms. Splenic mesenchyme exercising a phagocytic and digestive activity. Tc'. Tumor cells in various stages of disintegration. Tc" . Tumor cells transformed into blocks of almost structureless protein. V. Vessel. X. Tumor cells tightly surrounded by mesenchymal cells. The figures illustrate the result of growth of mixed grafts, consisting of mam- malian tumor and adult chick spleen on the allantois. PLATE I. FIG. i. Actively growing tumor focus of 4 days growth in the allantois. In the center groups of healthy tumor cells with numerous mitoses. The focus of healthy tumor tissue is surrounded by a zone in which the splenic mesenchymal cells encircle the tumor cells and digest them in closed vacuoles. FIG. 2. Small area of the peripheral zone around a tumor focus of 4 days growth. Only two tumor cells (H Tc) still exhibit a healthy structure. All the others (Tc' and Tc"} show various changes dependent upon the digestive activity of the splenic mesenchymal syncytium enclosing them in vacuoles. BIOLOGICAL BULLETIN VOL. XXXVIII. PLATE FIG .1. n * • r '••- • • •• . _, * «• * •/* * * . « V • • V- » * • Vr * * /•< "*" • • ^ ».v •\v; • •i> »'••*! • r ; -V* -V • •• . - V "" ;.' . . " ,-j .t--. ^ m : _,_' ' ' -;V.- v r>''"- "' '^ •-' ', • S.Ms. •>.% ' " i'j '*• • &. * ' • « . , t ." /-,. V v , «V» » • ^ • . * •' • * i' •' ^\;S^' ''v'^oV^, *ni :fA>'v **. c • ' v ^ ,v* i * « *».* . .** H.Tc. W ^ i ' ^ ., -*t *,', , S.Ms. ^ , j V. DANCHAKOFF. FIG. 2. 212 VERA DANCHAKOFF. PLATE II. FIG. 3. Small region of the peripheral zone around a tumor focus of 4 days growth, showing a group of healthy tumor cells (H Tc) and numerous other tumor cells in various stages of disintegration. F'iG. 4. Small area of the peripheral zone around a tumor focus of 4 days growth, illustrating the disintegration of the tumor cells within the vacuoles, formed by the splenic mesenchymal cells. BIOLOGICAL BULLETIN, VOL. XXXVMI FIG. 3. '*., 9 • £k* . . - ' - ' Tc.' S.Ms. - ' I.*,: V-- m - H.Tc. H.Tc. •" V ' •• .**• r i Tc.' Gr.Lc > ' Tc.' H.Tc. S.Ms. - ; Gr.Lc. d * •&. - Tc.' ™ .<**. V. OANCHAKOFF. FIG. 4- CHROMOSOME STUDIES IN TETTIGID^. II. CHRO- MOSOMES OF PARATETTIX BB AND CC AND THEIR HYBRID BC.1 MARY T. HARMAN. Introduction 2I3 Observations • 2I4 1. Spermatogonial Divisions 2I4 a. Chromosomes of CC 2I5 b. Chromosomes of BB 215 c. Chromosomes of BC 2I° 2. Growth Period 216 3. Spermatocyte Divisions • 2I7 4. The X-chromosome • 2I& Discussion . . 218 1. Synapsis and Heredity 2I9 2. Chromosomes in Hybrids • • • 221 Conclusions : Literature Cited • 223 Explanation of Plates • • • 226 INTRODUCTION. As was stated in a previous paper (Harman '15) the work on the cytological constitution of the germ-cells of Paratettix has been undertaken for the purpose of discovering whether or not the microscope will reveal any differences in the germ-cells of very closely related forms which may be correlated with the differences in hereditary characteristics. Three forms have been considered in this paper; two breeding true to type, BB and CC and their hybrid BC (Nabours '14 and '17). In his breeding work with Paratettix Nabours ('17) has con- sidered only the color patterns of the pronota and femora of the jumping legs. He has found fourteen distinct color patterns which behave as a unit and cannot further be broken up. These color patterns he designated as multiple allelomorphs. In most crossings between different pure types the resultant hybrid is readily discernible from either parent. In those hybrids which 1 Contribution from the Zoological Laboratory, Kansas State Agricultural Col- lege, No. 27. 213 214 MARY T. HARM AN. are not so readily discerned from the parents, a closer examina- tion shows that the pigment of each parent is distributed in about equal proportions. There is no evidence of dominance nor crossing over, and the proportions of offspring are almost per- fectly Mendelian even in small numbers. It is from these grouse locusts that the material for the present paper was obtained. The author wishes again to thank Dr. R. K. Nabours for this material and express her appreciation to him for furnishing her the material at a time when she could use it and for giving her access to his records from which she has been able to know the pedigrees of her animals. OBSERVATIONS. Only the male germ-cells have been studied. Observations have been made upon the spermatogonial divisions, the growth period, synapsis and the maturation divisions. Special con- sideration has been given to the metaphase plate, polar view, of the spermatogonial divisions, the behavior of the chromatin material during the growth period and the manner of the forma- tion of the first spermatocyte chromosomes. i . Spermatogonial Divisions. In all three forms the thirteen spermatogonial chromosomes in the metaphase are arranged on the spindle, having the appear- ance of the spokes on the hub of a wheel (Fig. i). As may be seen in a metaphase plate, polar view (Figs. 3 to 12), these chromosomes are in pairs and the members of each pair are approximately of equal size and of similar shape. The accessory chromosome is larger than the two smallest pairs and smaller than the other chromosomes. However, many times the differ- ence in size between the chromosomes of the second and third pairs and the X-chromosome is so slight in the spermatogonia that it is difficult to distinguish them. In Fig. 9, the homologous chromosomes of the different pairs are designated by the same number. In the anaphase the chromosomes take a position more in the same direction of the fibers of the spindle rather than at right angles to it as in the metaphase (Fig. 2). The change from the CHROMOSOME STUDIES IN TETTIGID/E. 215 perpendicular position in the metaphase to the parallel position in the anaphase would indicate a telomitic fiber attachment. In the late anaphase and early telophase the chromatin material begins to take on a granular appearance before the chromosomes have formed into a spireme (Fig. 13). In fact the cell has completed its division before there has been much reconstruction of the chromatin material. (a) Chromosomes of CC. — Figures 3, 4 and 5 are metaphase plates, polar view, of CC spermatogonial cells. Four of the thirteen chromosomes are decidedly larger than the others. The six smallest chromosomes are,,somewhat narrower at the proximal end than at the distal end. The third pair, marked 3 in these figures, are distinctly pointed at the proximal end, but there is no indication of a bend or hook on this pointed end. The X-chromo- some is more ovoid and when lying in certain positions shows a slight constriction in the middle. Figure 3 shows one of the larger chromosomes split at its distal end. This must be a precocious division, as it was not observed in any of the hundreds of other cells examined. Often one of the chromosomes is observed in the center of the spindle (Fig. 4), or the proximal end may be near the center (Fig. 5). (&) Chromosomes of BB. — In general the arrangement of the chromosomes of BB are like those of CC (Figs. 9-12). There are the two large pairs, the three smaller pairs and an intermediate pair. The X-chromosome is ovoid with a slight constriction near the middle (Fig. 9). There also occurs the crowding of the chromosomes toward the center of the spindle. The most evident difference between BB and CC is in the third pair, marked 3 in all the figures. Not only are these chromosomes pointed at their proximal ends, but the point is considerably bent to form a definite hook in BB. Sometimes this hook forms an acute angle with the other part of the chromosome (Fig. 12), but more often the bend is rounded (Fig. 10). The hooked chromosome on the left in figure 10 is much smaller than its homologue on the right, but this is due to the fact that a portion of its distal end was cut off as was also the large chromosomes on either side of it. These parts of chromosomes were found in the preceding section. 2l6 MARY T. HARMAN. (c) Chromosomes of BC. — The hybrid BC (Figs. 6-8) has only one hooked chromosome, while its homologue is merely pointed like in CC. This difference is constant in the many metaphase plates examined. There is some variation in the shape of the chromosomes and the size of the bend of the pointed end. In Fig. 6 the hooked member of the third pair is almost crescent shaped, while in the other cells the hooked chromosome is thick at the distal end, tapers toward the proximal end, and then bends abruptly. 2. Growth Period. Figures 13 to 24 illustrate the qhanges which take place in the growth period. At the end of the last spermatogonial divi- sions the chromosomes have a crinkled appearance. Figure 13 shows all thirteen of the chromosomes in this condition. How- ever the X-chromosome is more compact than the others. This crinkled appearance is due to the chromatin becoming less compact and more granular. There is no sign of a nuclear membrane nor any indication of a separation of the nuclear area from the cytoplasm. Soon the chromatin becomes very finely granular and the chromosomes unite end to end (Fig. 14). Shortly after this stage the chromatin has the appearance of a finely granular continuous thread and a light area separates the cytoplasm from the nuclear material (Figs. 15 and 16). The cell begins to increase in size and the chromatin thread becomes contracted into a ball (Fig. 17). There seems to be no definite polarization to the loops of the spireme, but it has more the appearance of a tangled mass. Sometimes it is with difficulty that the individual fibres are distinguished. Now both the nucleus and the entire cell increase greatly in size (Figs. 18-21) and the chromatin material forms into a thicker thread (Fig. 18). Then the chromatin thread becomes looser and the granules are more concentrated, particularly in places throughout the strand (Fig. 19). The portion of chromatin marked X never becomes as granular as the remainder of it. This chromatin is the sex- chromosome and can be identified in all the stages. Following the opening up of the tangled knot the chromatin thread breaks into pieces (Figs. 20 and 21). Figure 22 shows thirteen pieces, twelve of which are uniting in pairs by an end CHROMOSOME STUDIES IN TETTIGID/E. to end union. Figures 23 and 24 show successive stages in the formation of the primary spermatocyte chromosomes. As the chromosomes become more compact in appearance the nuclear membrane disappears and there is no definite boundary again between the nucleus and cytoplasm (Fig. 24). During all these changes there has not been the least appear- ance of a side by side pairing of the chromosomes, neither has there been any indication of a longitudinal splitting of the chro- matin material either as chromosomes or as a chromatin thread. With the breaking up of the chromatin thread always more than the haploid number of chromatin elements may be seen, and in many instances there is evidently the cliploid number. This surely justifies the conclusion that at the end of the growth period previous to the formation of the primary spermatocyte chromosomes there appears the diploid number of chromosomes and that the real pairing of the chromosomes does not take place in the spireme stage. It seems, also, very evident that the union of the chromosomes is by telosynapsis. As was previously stated (Harman '15), the dumb-bell shape of the chromosomes is not due to the manner of the division of the chromosomes, but is a result of the way they are formed. This shape is noticed in some of the chromosomes even before the entire chromatin thread is broken up (Fig. 21), and certainly before the first spermatocyte spindle is formed (Fig. 24). 3. Spermatocyte Divisions. There are six dumb-bell shaped bivalent chromosomes and an ovoid univalent chromosome in the primary spermatocyte. These arrange themselves longitudinally on a large spindle (Figs. 25 and 26). The univalent, or X-chromosome, lies near the periphery of the spindle. The other chromosomes are crowded toward the center. Most often they are so closely crowded together that it is difficult to see them all at once in a lateral view of the spindle. Always one of the medium-sized chromo- somes lies very near the second-largest chromosome (Figs. 26 and 30). In the first division all the bivalent chromosomes divide at the constricted part of the dumb-bell (Fig. 27). In other words, 2l8 MARY T. HARMAN. they have divided at the point of union as we have previously seen. As the result of this division the chromosomes of the anaphase are ovoid. The X-chromosome does not divide in this division but passes to one pole much in advance of the others. The first division then is truly a reductional division. The spindle of the secondary spermatocyte is nearly spherical. Early in the metaphase the chromosomes show a longitudinal split (Figs. 28 and 29). All of the chromosomes divide in this division. It is an equational division in the sense of the dividing of the chromatin material of original chromosomes into halves. 4. The X-Chromosome. The X-chromosome is a persistent portion of chromatin ma- terial which can easily be identified throughout the growth period. It never becomes finely granular, nor does it have the woolly appearance of the other chromatin elements. The other chromo- somes lose their identity in the coiled thread, but the X-chromo- some does not form a part of this coiled knot. With the formation of the primary spermatocyte chromosomes it may be distin- guished from the others both by its shape and by its more compact appearance. On the primary spermatocyte spindle it is often found half way to the pole before the other chromosomes have completely divided. It is more difficult to recognize it in the secondary spermatocyte because all the chromosomes here are ovoid in shape, and the differences in size between some of the chromospmes are so much less. It behaves similarly to the other chromosomes here, neither being precocious in its division nor lagging. DISCUSSION. Concerning many of the most obvious and essential points of the behavior of the chromatin in the maturation of the sperm, there is no longer any debate. It is agreed that during this period the number of chromosomes is reduced to one half. That this reduction in number is brought about by the union of chro- mosomes by pairs is also generally accepted. As to how this pairing has taken place has been the subject of much discussion, and upon the answer to this question depends the acceptance or rejection of other theories. It seems to the writer that when we CHROMOSOME STUDIES IN TETTIGID.E. 2 19 have accepted the thought that this reduction in number takes place by the union of chromosomes in pairs we have essentially also accepted the theory of the individuality of chromosomes. Then how do these individuals come together and finally separate again? i . Synapsis and Heredity. One can scarcely follow out the discussion of Janssen on Batracoseps attennatus, and more especially when close attention is given to his drawings, without coming to the conclusion that with this form, at least, the chromosomes unite by parasynapsis and that it is perfectly possible in the twisting of certain chroma- tin elements to have an exchange of parts of homologous chromo- somes, thus forming a convenient mechanism for the "crossing over" of hereditary characteristics if we accept the chromosome hypothesis of heredity. The "chiasmatypie" theory of Janssen ('09) furnishes a very convenient means for explaining some ratios in Mendelian inheritance. An application of this chiasmatype theory, which should not pass without mention, is the work on Drosophila, particularly the work of Morgan and his students ('15). From the behavior of the hereditary characteristics a chromosome map has been constructed in which not only are the determiners of the char- acteristics located on definite chromosomes, but also the relative distances that these genes are from each other, is given. The position of the genes on the chromosomes is calculated from the percentages of cross-overs. In addition to the vast amount of genetic evidence for this condition in Drosophila there is also some cytological evidence. Metz ('14) in dealing with five different types of chromosome groups of Drosophila shows that the chromosomes exhibit a close association in pairs at nearly all times and that before each cell division the pairs become so intimately associated that they may be said actually to conjugate. He further states that the union of the chromosomes is "unquestionably a side-by- side, or parasynapsis one." In a later paper (Metz '16) the same author in considering chromosome pairing in about eighty species of Diptera, many of which belong to the genus Drosophila, states that the pairing "certainly involves the essential features of a synaptic (parasynaptic) union." 22O MARY T. HARMAN. Bridges ('17) confirmed the observations of Metz as to the spermatogonial and oogonial pairing, but deduced the kind of synapsis from his genetic data. The genetic behavior of Paratettix is very different from that of Drosopliila. In the hundreds of matings of Paratettix not a single instance of crossing-over has occurred. In accounting for the hereditary behavior of Parateltix without a knowledge of the cytological behavior Morgan has suggested the theory of identical loci. As has been previously shown, at no time during the growth period is there any indication of a double thread, and also there is evidence of an end to end union of the chromo- somes to form the tangled thread of the contraction figure. Moreover at the end of the growth period the chromatin material forms the diploid number of chromatin elements. Also these chromatin elements agree in relative sizes to the spermatogonial chromosomes. Undoubtedly these parts unite end to end. In this case there is little chance for crossing-over, as only the ends of homologous chromosomes come in contact with one another. In view then of the cytological behavior, it seems more likely that the series of multiple allelomorphs may be accounted for by the kind of synapsis rather than by the theory of identical loci. The writer is well aware that there is not perfect agreement as to how synapsis takes place in the Orthoptera, and even within a single family of the Orthoptera. McClung ('14) has recently reviewed the literature on the subject of synapsis in Orthoptera, and Wenrich ('16) has summarized the results. Briefly those who have described or assumed telosynapsis are: Montgomery ('05), Syrbula; Stevens ('05), Stenopalmatus, ('05) Blatta, ('10) Forficula; Wassilieff ('07), Blatta; Zweiger ('06), Forficula; Davis ('08), Acrididae and Locustidse; Buchner ('09), Gryllus, CEdipoda; Brunelli ('09), Gryllus., and ('10) Tryxalis; Sutton ('02, ''03), Brachystola; Baumgartner ('04), Gryllus; McClung ('05, '08, '14), various Orthoptera; Nowlin ('08), Melanoplus; Pinney ('08), Phrynotettix; Robertson ('08), Syrbula; Carothers ('13), Acrididae. Those who have assumed or described para- synapsis are: Gerard ('09), Stenobothrus ; Morse ('09), Blattidse; Stevens ('12), Ceuthophilus; Robertson ('15), Tettigidae; Ve- jodvsky ('11-12), Locustidae; and Otte ('07), Locusta. In addi- CHROMOSOME STUDIES IN TETTIGID^E. 221 tion to these Wenrich ('16) gives strong evidence for parasynapsis in Phrynotettix and ('17) Chorthippus and Tr inter otro pis. May it not be that within the order Orthoptera, or even within the same family, that the kind of synapsis is different? The behavior in heredity is certainly different. The behavior of Paratettix in heredity has already been discussed. Bellamy ('17) has described a similar condition in the genus Tettigidea. He found five allelomorphic color patterns without any indication of cross-overs. The cytological constitution of the germ-cells of his material has not been examined. However, Robertson ('17) has described parasynapsis in Tettigidea parvipennis. Nabours ('19) has not only found crossing-over in Apotettix, but has also found parthenogenesis and crossing-over in those repro- ducing parthenogenetically. Certainly if we accept the chromo- some hypothesis of heredity there must be a difference in the cytological behavior of the germ-cells of these genera. Certainly enough has already been said in this paper to show that the chro- mosomes pair by telosynapsis in Paratettix. With the many cross-overs in Apotettix, and the similar behavior in inheritance to Drosophila, one would expect the chromosome behavior to be different than in Paratettix and more like Drosophila. The writer has not yet studied the chromatin behavior in Apotettix. 2. Chromosomes in Hybrids. In working with the chromosomes of hybrids Moenkhaus, Morris, Federley, Harrison and Doncaster, Pinney and others have found that despite the fact that the paternal chromosomes are in a foreign cytoplasm they retain their characteristic form, size and number. Moenkhaus ('04) pointed out that when Fundulus is crossed with Menidia two kinds of chromosomes are present in the fertilized egg and can readily be distinguished in later divisions, furthermore, that these two kinds of chromo- somes are like the chromosomes of each parent respectively. Morris ('14) found two types of chromosomes in the early cleavage stages of the hybrid of Fundulus heteroclitus 9 and Ctenolabrus adspersus c? . These types of chromosomes she identifies as the two types of parental chromosomes. Pinney (' 1 8) made a number of crosses with teleosts and also 222 MARY T. HARMAN. made the reciprocal crosses. She found that some cytoplasm seemed to be more favorable to foreign sperm than others. In some instances a few chromosomes were eliminated, but those that did remain could be identified as to their paternal or maternal origin. Similar results were obtained by Federley ('13) and Harrison and Doncaster ('14) in hybrids of certain moths. In the above cases cited there is no indication that these hybrids may occur in nature, and these offspring were obtained from animals which are accepted as distinct species. In the case of Paratettix, P. BB and P. CC are not yet admitted to be species by taxonomists, and they are known to cross in nature, judging from hybrids obtained in the wild and segregated in the laboratory. The chromosomes of each form are so very similar to those of the other forms in so many ways that it is only with close study that the differences are recognized. Yet, when once recognized it is found to be constant. The chromosomes of the hybrids of Paratettix are different from those of the fish mentioned above in that the parental chromosomes are more alike and that the cytoplasm of the egg seems to be perfectly compatible with the chromatin of the foreign sperm. They are different from those of the moths in that there are the same number of chromo- somes in both forms and there is a complete synapsis with the homologous pairs of chromosomes. CONCLUSIONS. 1. The third pair of spermatogonial chromosomes of BB are bent at the proximal end so as to form distinct hooks. 2. The third pair of spermatogonial chromosomes of CC are pointed at the proximal end, but there is no bend. 3. The third pair of spermatogonial chromosomes of the hybrid, BC, is composed of one hooked member and one pointed member. 4. During the growth period there is no indication of a parallel condition of the chromatin either in the chromatin thread or in the chromosomes. 5. At the end of the growth period there is evidently the diploid number of chromosomes formed which correspond in relative sizes to the respective spermatogonial chromosomes. 6. Synapsis does not take place in the thread but at the end CHROMOSOME STUDIES IN TETTIGID^. 223 of the growh period by an end to end union of the homologous pairs of chromosomes. 7. The chromatin of the sex-chromosomes does not lose its identity during the growth period. 8. The first maturation division is a reductional division, and the second maturation division is equational. 9. The formation of the diploid number of chromosomes at the end of the growth period and the union of their homologous pairs by telosynapsis may explain the absence of any crossing- over in Paratettix. LITERATURE CITED. Baumgartner, W. J. '04 Some New Evidence for the Individuality of Chromosomes. BIOL. BULL., Vol. 8. Bellamy, A. W. '17 Studies of Inheritance and Evolution in Orthoptera, IV. Multiple Allelo- morphism and Inheritance of Color Patterns in Tettigidea. Jour. Gen., Vol. 7. Bridges, Calvin B. '17 Non-disjunction as a Proof of the Chromosome Theory of Heredity. Gen ., Vol. i. Brunelli, G. '09 La spermatogenesi del Gryllus desertus Pall. (Division! spermatogoniali e maturative.) Reale Academia dei Lincei, T. 2, serie sa. '10 La spermatogenesi della Tryxalis. Division! spermatogoniali. Memarie della Societa Italiana Scienze, T. 16, serie 33. Buchner, P. '09 Das accessorische Chromosom in Spermatogenese und Ovogenese der Orthopteran, Zugleich ein Beitrag zur Kenntnis der Reduktion. Archiv. f. Zellforsh., Bd. 3. Carothers, E. Eleanor. '13 The Mendelian Ratio in Relation to Certain Orthopteran Chromosomes. Jour. Morph., Vol. 24. Davis, H. S. '08 Spermatogenesis in Acrididse and Locustidse. Bull. M. C. Z., Vol. 53. Federley, H. '13 Das Verhalten der Chromosomen bei der Spermatogenese der Schmettalinge, etc. Zeit. Abst. Vererb., Bd. 9. Gerard, P. '09 Recherches la Spermatogenese Chez Stenobothrus bigutulus (Linn.). Archives de Biologie, T. 24. Harman, Mary T. '15 Spermatogenesis in Paratettix. BIOL. BULL., Vol. 29. Harrison, J. W. H., and Doncaster, L. '14 On Hybrids between Moths of the Geometrid Subfamily Bistoninse, with an Account of the Behavior of the Chromosomes in Gametogenesis in Lycia (Biston) historic,, Ithysia (Myssia) Zonaria and in their Hybrids. Jour. Gen., Vol. 3. 224 MARY T. HARMAN. Janssens, F. A. '09 La theorie de la chiasmatypie Nouvelle interpretation des cineses de maturation. La Cellule, T. 25. McClung, C. E. '05 The Chromosome Complex of Orthopteran Spermatocytes. BIOL. BULL., Vol. 9. '08 The Spermatogenesis of 'X.iphidium faciatum. Kansas Univ. Sci. Bull., Vol. 4. '14 A Comparative Study of Chromosomes in Orthopteran Spermatogenesis. Jour. Morph., Vol. 25. Metz, Charles W. '14 Chromosome Studies in Diptera I. A Preliminary Survey of Five Different Types of Chromosome Groups in the Genus Drosophila. Jour. Exp. Zool., Vol. 17. '16 Chromosome Studies on the Diptera II. The Paired Association of Chromosomes in the Diptera and its Significance. Jour. Exp. Zool., Vol. 21. Moenkhaus, Wm. J. '04 The Development of the Hybrid between Fundulus heteroclUus and Menidia nolata with Especial Reference to the Behavior of the Maternal and Paternal Chromatin. Am. Jour. Anat., Vol. 3. Montgomery, T. H., Jr. '05 The Spermatogenesis of Syrbula and Sycosa, with General Considerations upon Chromosome Reduction and the Heterochromosomes. Proc. Acad- emy Nat. Sci. Philadelphia, February. Morgan, T. H. and others. '15 The Mechanism of Mendelian Heredity. Henry Holt and Co. New York. Morris, Margaret. '14 The Behavior of the Chromatin in Hybrids between Fundulus and Cteno- labnis. Jour. Exp. Zool., Vol. 16. Morse, Max. '09 The Nuclear Components of the Sex Cells of Four Species of Cockroaches. Archiv. f. Zellforsch., Bd. 3. Nabours, Robert K. '14 Studies of Inheritance and Evolution in Orthoptera I. Jour. Gen., Vol. 3. '17 Studies of Inheritance and Evolution in Orthoptera II, III. Jour. Gen., Vol. 7. '19 Parthenogenesis and Crossing-over in the Grouse Locust Apotettix. Amer. Nat., Vol. 53. Otte, H. '07 Samenreifung und Samenbildung bei Locusta viridissima. Zool. Jahrb. Abt. f. Anat., Bd. 24. Pinney, Edith. '08 Organization of the Chromosomes in Phrynoteltix magnus. Kansas Univ. Sci. Bull., Vol. 4. "18 A Study of the Relation of the Behavior of the Chromatin to Development and Heredity in Teleost Hybrids. Jour. Morph., Vol. 31. Robertson, W. Rees Brebner. '08 The Chromosome Complex of Syrbula admirabilis. Kansas Univ. Sci. Bull., Vol. 4. CHROMOSOME STUDIES IN TETTIGID.E. 225 '15 Inequalities and Deficiencies in Homologous Chromosomes; their Bearing upon Synapsis and the Loss of Unit Characters. Jour. Morph., Vol. 26. '17 Chromosome Studies IV. A Deficient Supernumerary Accessory Chromo- some in a Male of Tettigidea parvipennis. Kansas Univ. Sci. Bull., \ ol. 10. Stevens, N. M. '05 Studies in Spermatogenesis with Especial Reference to the "Accessory Chromosome." Carnegie Inst. Publ. 36. '10 An Unequal Pair of Heterochromosomes in Forficula. Jour. Exp. Zool., Vol. 8. '12 Supernumerary Chromosomes and Synapsis in Ceuthophilus. BIOL. BULL., 22. Sutton, W. S. '02 On the Morphology of the Chromosome-group in Brachyslola magna. BIOL. BULL., Vol. 4. '08 The Chromosomes in Heredity. BIOL. BULL., Vol. 4. Vejdovsky, F. '12 Zum Problem der Vererbungstrager. Bohm. Gesell. wiss., Prag. Wassilieff, A. '07 Die Spermatogenese von Blatta germanica. Archiv. f. Mikr. Anat., Bd. 70. Wenrich, D. H. '16 The Spermatogenesis in Phyrnotettix magnus, with Special Reference to Synapsis and the Individuality of the Chromosomes. Bull. Mus. Comp. Zool., Vol. 60. '17 Synapsis and Chromosome Organization in Charthippus (Stenobothrus) curtipennis and Trimerotropis suffusa (Orthoptera). Jour. Morph., Vol. 29. Zweiger, H. '06 Die Spermatogenese von Forficula auricularia L. Jena. Zeit., Bd. 42 226 MARY T. HARMAN. EXPLANATION OF PLATES. All figures were made at table level by means of a Zeiss compensating ocular No. 6 and a 1.5 objective with the aid of a camera lucida. Figures 13, 14 and 15 were enlarged three and one half diameters. All others were enlarged two and one half diameters. All were reduced one third. PLATE I. FIG. i. BC, metaphase plate, lateral view, of spermatogonial division. Fig. 2. BC, late anaphase, spermatogonial division. FIG. 3. CC, metaphase plate, polar view, spermatogonial division; X is the odd-chromosome, 3, 3 is the third pair of chromosomes, and 2 is one of the chromo- somes of the fifth pair showing a precocious split. FIGS. 4 AND 5. CC, metaphase plates, polar view, spermatogonial divisions; 3, 3 third pair of chromosomes. FIGS. 6, 7 AND 8. BC, metaphase plates, polar view, spermatogonial divisions; 3, 31 third pair of chromosomes, 31 is the hooked chromosome of the third pair. FIG. 9. BB, metaphase plate, polar view, spermatogonial division; the numbers i, 1-2, 2, etc., to 6, 6 are homologous pairs of chromosomes, X is the odd-chromo- some. It will be noted that the members of the third pair are both hooked chromo- somes in BB. FIGS. 10, ii AND 12. BB, metaphase plates, polar view of spermatogonial divisions, 3, 3 is the third pair of chromosomes. BIOLOGICAL BULLETIN, VOL. XXXVIII. PLATE I. 9 M. T. HARMAN 228 MARY T. HARMAN. PLATE II. FIG. 13. Early telophase of last spermatogonial division. X, the odd-chromo- some. FIG. 14. Beginning of growth period showing the uniting of the chromosomes to form spireme; X is chromatin of the odd-chromosome in all the figures of this plate. FIG. 15. Early loose chromatin thread. FIG. 16. Early contraction stage. FIG. 17. Later contraction stage and the appearance of the nuclear membrane. FIG. 18.' Large tangled thread. FIG. 19. Beginning of the concentration of the chromatin in various places throughout the thread. FIG. 20. Breaking up of chromatin thread into the diploid number of chromatin elements. FIG. 21. Further breaking of the chromatin thread and the beginning of the formation of the bivalent chromosomes. BIOLOGICAL BULLETIN, VOL. XXXVII 16 W¥P I T fev'' ..;.«?&..**. ., ^ ...;-^M_ ; , 18 &•'***«- ""vX/^'V*. 20 M. T. HARMAN. 230 MARY T. HARMAN. PLATE III. FIG. 22. Union of univalent chromatin elements to form bivalent chromo- somes. X is the odd-chromosome in all figures of this plate. FIG. 23. Bivalent chromosomes with the nuclear membrane present. FIG. 24. Bivalent chromosomes before the formation on the spindle. The two elements of the third chromosome have not yet united. FIG. 25. First spermatocyte spindle showing the seven chromosomes. FIG. 26. Metaphase, lateral view of first spermatocyte division. FIG. 27. Early anaphase of first spermatocyte division. The odd-chromosome approaching one pole in advance of the other chromosomes. FIG. 28. Metaphase plate, lateral view of second spermatocyte division. Four of the chromosomes show the longitudinal split. FIG. 29. Metaphase plate, polar view of second spermatocyte division. FIG. 30. Metaphase plate, polar view of first spermatocyte division. BIOLOGICAL BULLETIN, VOL. XXXVIII. PIAIf III. 30 M. T. HARMAN. WHITE-OCELLI— AN EXAMPLE OF A "SLIGHT' MU- TANT CHARACTER WITH NORMAL VIABILITY. CALVIN B. BRIDGES. Most of the non-lethal mutant characters of Drosophila melano- gaster may be described as "slight" in the amount of visible change. This is fortunate, for, in general, the mutant genes that cause great changes in an organ, or that cause several distinct changes in different organs, produce races of poorer viability than the wild type of the species. The amount of disturbance to viability is roughly proportional to the extent of the somatic change. In nature these "extreme" mutant types would not be able to survive in competition with the wild type, and in our experimental cultures their presence leads to aberrant ratios that tend to obscure the simple genetic relations. It is possible to reduce the shortage of these relatively inviable mutant characters only by making the conditions of culture exceptionally favorable. Thus, the number of eggs per culture must be re- stricted by breeding from a single mother; and the amount of food per culture should be carefully regulated. Great progress has been made in improving the quality and methods of using culture media. While it is possible by such improved methods to make the relatively inviable mutants usable in many kinds of experiments, there remain other experiments that require so high a degree of exactness or the simultaneous use of so many mutants that even one poorly viable mutant is inadmissible. As the number of mutant types increases, we are able to drop the use of more and more of the poorly viable mutants and to replace them by new mutants of normal viability. As already implied, these mutants of normal viability are nearly all characterized by somatic changes of slight degree, or by effects, so far as observ- able, upon a single organ only. Many of these "slight" char- acters are perfectly definite in demarkation and offer as great precision of classification as do more extreme mutant characters. Another great advantage of slight mutant characters is that they 231 232 CALVIN B. BRIDGES. do not preclude the simultaneous use of other mutants. On the other hand, the use of such an extreme mutant character as white-eyes prevents the effective use in the same experiment of all other eye colors, and likewise the use of vestigial-wing hinders the use of all other wing and venation characters. The most valuable mutants are, then, those of slight but definite somatic change, free from disturbance to viability or masking effects on other mutant characters. ORIGIN AND DESCRIPTION OF THE WHITE-OCELLI MUTANT. One of the best examples of a slight mutant character that fulfills these conditions is that of "white-ocelli," which was found very early in the breeding work with Drosophila (June 21, 1912). The ocelli of wild flies are three small simple eyes in a group on the dorsal posterior part of the head. In color they are of a dilute brownish-red, about that of a coffee infusion. Close examination shows that the color of the ocelli themselves is quite light, about that of weak tea, but that there is a crescent" shaped deposit of dense brownish-red pigment about the median side of the two posterior ocelli and against the posterior side of the anterior ocellus. The apparent color of the ocellus is largely due to this outside pigment seen through the transparent lens-like ocellus, and consequently the color changes in intensity with the angle at which the ocellus is viewed. It was observed that the ocelli of white and also of vermilion- eyed flies were without color or pigment deposit. These ocellar changes are only other effects of the white and vermilion genes. That the color of the ocelli could vary independently of that of the eyes became apparent when it was found that about half of the flies of the stock of the mutant black-body color had white ocelli, while the remainder had the normal coffee-colored ocelli. Some of the white-ocelli flies were bred together and gave a pure-breeding stock of white-ocelli flies (June, 1912). About a year after this, it was found (July, 1913) that the stock of the third-chromosome recessive spineless was also pure for the white-ocellar color. The gene for white-ocelli was thought to be in the third chromosome, since, in crosses in which the spineless was used, the white-ocellar character generally reap- peared in association with the spineless, though sometimes not. WHITE-OCELLI. 233 The usefulness of the mutant white-ocelli was not appreciated for some years after its discovery. This neglect was due, in large part, to the fact that the regular examination of flies during this period was carried out by aid of a hand lens only, and the separation of the white from the normal ocelli was difficult because of the small size of the region affected. The later work has been done with a binocular microscope, with special attention to proper illumination and magnification; and under these condi- tions the separation is complete and entirely accurate, though still somewhat slow. THE LINKAGE RELATIONS OF WHITE-OCELLI. An accurate localization of the gene for white-ocelli was made easy by the use of the two excellent dominant characters dichsete and hairless. The locus of dichaete was near the left end of the map of the third chromosome as then known, being about 13 units to the right of sepia, while the locus of hairless was some- what to the right of the middle (about 42 units to the right of sepia and 21 to the left of rough). Spineless white-ocelli males were outcrossed to dichaete hairless females; and the FI dichsete hairless females were back-crossed by spineless white-ocelli males, with the results shown below: o . I K. 2 ; . i, 2, i, 3- 2, 3- 1918 D D D D D D D Sept. ss ss ss ss ss ss SS Total 27 H H H H H H H \vo wo wo wo wo wo wo Total 660 528 88 80 116 87 So S3 5 4 2 8 o I 1,682 * Crossovers in the first region, that between dichsete and spineless, are headed by " i," etc. The results of this experiment showed that the locus of white- ocelli is to the right of that of hairless by about 6.8 units (a total of 114 crossovers involving region three). The locus of white- ocelli, as thus established, is in what had been the longest un- occupied region of the third chromosome. There had been no workable mutant in the entire distance of about 20 units from ebony (1.5 units to the right of hairless) to rough (21.2 units to 234 CALVIN B. BRIDGES. the right of hairless). The ebony rough distance was so great that in constructing a map a correction was required on account of double crossing over. The presence of white-ocelli between ebony and rough gave an opportunity to make a direct test of the amount of double crossing over and consequently of the amount of correction required. The results of the spineless white-ocelli X hairless rough back-cross are given below: o 2 • I, 3- 2, 3- 1919 SS SS SS SS SS SS Feb. H H H H H H Total 25 WO wo wo wo WO wo ro ro ro ro ro ro Total.. 1,084 1.126 16? TO=; TTS OT ?.?,f> 2^ TT o 0 T T.I7Q There was 6.6 per cent, of crossing over between hairless and white-ocelli, which agrees with the value 6.8 found in the previous experiment. Likewise, the white-ocelli rough value of 15.8 is in agreement with the expectation from the usual value of 22 for hairless rough. There was only one double crossover in the hairless white-ocelli rough section — a percentage relatively very low. Comparisons show that in the third chromosome (as in the second) the region near the end of the chromosome has a far lower 'coincidence' than has the mid-region. The amount of correction of the observed crossover value for the hairless ebony interval is thus .063 per cent., or somewhat less than one tenth of one unit. Other back-cross tests involving this region have produced a total of 45,971 flies, of which 19.6 per cent, were crossovers. This value is to be corrected to 19.7, which is the map-distance between ebony and rough. THE VIABILITY OF THE WHITE-OCELLI MUTANT. As just seen, the linkage of white-ocelli was worked out through use of the more convenient spineless white-ocelli stock, while the black white-ocelli stock was discarded. The white- ocelli character persisted in the original black stock. No effort was made to eliminate it, nor, on the other hand, to aid in its survival. In May, 1919, a census of the flies of this black stock showed that approximately half were white-ocellars. That is, WHITE-OCELLI. 235 the character had persisted in undiminished frequency from June, 1912, to May, 1919, a period that represented fully 175 genera- tions of flies. During this period the black stock had been carried on in mass-cultures. Every two weeks a new culture was started by transferring, without examination or selection, enough flies to insure breeding. In such mass-cultures overcrowding is ex- treme, and, in spite of. the great numbers of parents, not many more offspring succeed in hatching than hatch from successful pair-cultures. The competition grows keener with the age of the culture, since the number of larvae is continually increasing from eggs laid each day, while the quantity of available food soon begins to diminish and its quality becomes progressively poorer. The mass-culture method of breeding thus exercises a strong and continuous selection against the perpetuation of the weaker or slower hatching individuals or types. In several instances mixed stocks have been started with equal numbers of different muta- tions, and this stock transferred without selection through several generations. Watch was kept, and in these cases there has been a progressive change in the composition of the stock, rapid at first, until the numbers of one type were quite small, and there- after slower but in the same direction. Recessive characters of very low viability may persist for many generations as a small proportion of the population. Their existence is maintained by the inter-crossing of the heterozygotes, whereby the mutant gene escapes the adverse selection that the mutant character suffers. Certain of our mutations are so sensitive to larval overcrowding that the ratios in mass-cultures and in pair-cultures seem to belong to different systems of heredity. Thus, the character strap approaches I in 4 in pair-cultures, but may approximate I in 1 6 in sister mass-cultures. The persistence of the white-ocelli character in undiminished proportion through 175 generations of forced competition means that the mutant is under no disadvantage. Such a mutant might easily survive in nature, and one slightly advantageous might ultimately supplant the original type. 236 CALVIN B. BRIDGES. THE MODIFICATION OF EOSIN EYE COLOR BY WHITE-OCELLI. An examination of the various stocks of eye color mutations showed that there was a strong correlation between the eye color and the ocellar color. The ocelli of white-eyed flies are entirely colorless. The ocelli of vermilion-eyed flies show a slight trace only of color. Indeed, in the case of vermilion, the vermilion gene has a relatively greater effect upon ocellar color than upon eye color. The ocellar color of pink is so faint that pink can not be used in the same experiment with white-ocelli without some confusion in classification. The ocellar color of the dark eye 'sepia' is itself also darker. In the ten multiple allelomorphs of the white series, the ocellar color is proportional to the eye color. This direct effect of eye color genes on ocellar color suggested that the reverse relation might also hold — namely, that the white-ocelli gene might dilute the eye color. A careful examination of the eye color of white-ocelli flies did not show any certain effect. White-ocelli was crossed to vermilion, and the p2 vermilion white-ocelli flies were not distinguishable from the simple vermilion flies. In the ¥2 of the cross between white- ocelli and eosin, a definite modification of the eosin by the white- ocelli gene was observed. In the case of the males, the eye color of the double form was lighter in intensity and less yellow in tone than that of the eosin brothers. In the females, the change was in the same direction but was less marked in degree. Probably 95 per cent, of the diluted males were separable from the simple eosin, while only about 60 per cent, of the females were thus separable. Eosin is known to be especially subject to specific modification,1 and the effects of the white-ocellar gene give a color intensity and tone and a sexual difference practically identical with those observed in the case of the modifier 'pinkish.' The gene for pinkish was, however, in the second chromosome, and there are other differences between the two cases. Jour. Exp. Zoo/., July, 1919. OBSERVATIONS ON THE SEXUAL CYCLE OF THE GUINEA PIG. O. ISHII, PATHOLOGICAL LABORATORY OF THE WESTERN PENNSYLVANIA HOSPITAL, PITTSBURGH, PA. In the following work, which was undertaken at the suggestion of Professor Leo Loeb, my aim has been to correlate the micro- scopical and experimental analysis of the sexual cycle which this author had previously made and further studies which he had planned at that time with a careful study of the cyclic changes as far as they are accessible to the naked eye. These observations were made at the breeding establishment of Miss A. E. C. Lathrop, in Granby, Mass., during the years 1913 and 1914. l THE THREE STAGES OF MENSTRUATION AND HEAT. i . Prcestrum. — This period continues for one to one and a half days, during which time the external genital organs become congested and swollen and a slight serous secretion is noticeable. This secretion, while in some instances turbid or viscid, is in the majority of cases more or less transparent; while, as before said, this period averages one to one and a half days, in some instances it may continue for two or three days or more. In the last 4 or 5 hours toward the end of this period a maximum in these changes is reached. This is followed by the period of heat. The duration of the procestrus period is indicated on the following table. Ninety-four (94) animals served for this observation. Length of Procestrus Period. 1A Day. i Day. i% Day 2 Days. 3 Days. 4 Days. No of guinea pigs I 39 34 II 8 1 1 Accidental conditions greatly delayed the publication of this paper. In the meantime there has appeared a study of Stockard and Papanicolaou on the oestrous cycle of the guinea pig. Notwithstanding this long delay in publication, we believe that our observations present new facts of interest and are worth recording. 237 238 O. ISHII. On the basis of these observations it may be reasonably said that the average length of this period in an animal of normal physical condition is from one to one and a half day, but after puerperium, or in case of weakness or of pathologic conditions, a shortening or prolongation may be brought about. 2. (Estrus, or Heat. — The approach of this period is made apparent by excessive menstrual secretion during the last 4 or 5 hours of proffistrum. This continues for 6 or 7 hours of the oestrus period. This is followed by the" secretion of a small quantity of moisture which may be just noticeable, or during the last 3 or 4 hours of the oestrus period menstrual secretion may be lacking altogether; still heat is present in this period. I give the following data as to the length of period of heat: Length of Heat Period. 5 to 6 Hrs. 7 to 8 Hrs. 1 9 to 10 Hrs. ii to 12 Hrs. 13 to 14 Hrs No. of guinea pigs . 6 51 62 30 9 As can be seen from the above figures, from 8 to n hours is the average length of the heat period in the healthy animal. I found those guinea pigs in which the periods were longer (12 to 14 hours) or shorter (5 to 6 hours) to have been lacking in vigor or to have shown abnormal physical conditions. In the normal animal I feel justified in claiming, on the average, 9 to n hours as the correct length of this period in the normal guinea pig. 3. Metcestrum. — This is the period following the oestrus period and might be considered as the period of recovery from the cestrous changes. The length of this period depends almost wholly on the condition of the animal. In the normal animal 2^ to 3 days are required until the normal condition is re- established. From the figures that follow it will be noticed that the length of this period varies from i^ to 5 days, but my observations have convinced me that where the recovery is shorter or longer than the average period, the animal is not in a normal healthy condition. Length of Metoestrus Period. *% Days. 2 Days. 2^ Days. 3 Days. 31A Days. 4 Days. 4^ Days. 5 Days. No. of guinea pigs. . . . 4 9 30 2O 15 10 5 4 SEXUAL CYCLE OF THE GUINEA PIG. 239 I may state that at this penod a slightly transparent serous or a turbid or viscid secretion can be noticed; but within 2 or 3 days this will disappear, and the swelling of the external organ will cease and normal conditions will again prevail. PHENOMENA OF FIRST MENSTRUATION OR HEAT PERIOD IN THE GUINEA PIG. The first heat or menstruation of the guinea pig differs in some respects from the later periods of heat. The symptoms of prooestrum are of much longer duration. While as stated above, the normal prooestrum lasts about I to 1^2 days, during the first menstruation a slight secretion and swelling continues for 3 to 8 days; only then appears the period of heat proper. In some instances the secretion and swelling disappear temporarily. Then a few days later it begins again and now develops into the regular heat. As stated, generally the duration of the procestrum is variable and usually longer in the first menstrual period. DICESTRUS PERIOD OF THE GUINEA PIG. The dioestrus period is the length of time between two consecu- tive menstruations or heat periods. Through observation of the condition of heat we determined the length of the sexual cycle in the guinea pig; we used for this purpose a considerably larger material than Loeb1 and Stockard and Papanicoloau2 used in their observations. While our average agrees with that of the aforementioned authors, we found a greater range of variation than Stockard and Papanicolaou. From the observations of Loeb it follows that in some exceptional cases the period may be even shorter than that found by us. • Length of Time Between Heat. 1454 Days. Days. IS1/! Days. 16 Days. 16^ Days. *7 Days. 17^ Days. 18 Days. No., of guinea pigs .... I 40 72 62 36 12 8 I From these figures we may conclude that the average duration of this period was 15 to 16^2 days. I found that where the length of time was decidely shorter (14.1/2 days) or longer (17 to 1 Leo Loeb, BIOLOGICAL BULLETIN, 1914, XXVII. , i. 2 C. R. Stockard and G. Papanicolaou, Am. Jour. Anat., 1917, XXII., 225. 24O O. ISHII. 1 8 days) than the average, the animals were either in a weak or pathological condition, and that menstrual irregularities sub- sequently followed. My continuous experiments made on this subject covered both winter and summer of 1913, and the winter of 1914 in Granby, Mass., and during all these seasons, through all temperatures, the same condition prevailed. GENERAL PHENOMENA OF MENSTRUATION AND HEAT AND PREGNANCY. Physical Phenomena of Menstrual Period. One of the foremost indications of the approach of the period of menstruation or heat is the physical change noticed in the animal. The muscles and joints become tender and relaxed, and by this change it can be determined, whether the animal is normal and healthy. During this period nervousness and a certain mental depression are noticeable; and when the period is over the symptoms most apparent indicate fatigue or exhaustion. However, by the end of the second or third day this fatigue has entirely disappeared and within I ^ to 2 days after cessation of heat a normal animal has regained its full vitality. Relation between Menstruation and Falling Out of Hair. I have found, particularly in the guinea pig, rat and rabbit an additional visible sign of heat, viz., the falling out of the hair in unusual quantities during the period of menstruation; as a result of this happening, the external appearance of the animal becomes somewhat glossy. We can determine this by comparing the ease with which the hair can be made to fall off by rubbing the skin in the menstruating animal, on the one hand, and in the infantile, pregnant, amcestrous female or male, on the other hand. In exceptional cases a slight menstrual secretion may develop during pregnancy; in this case the falling out of the hair is slight in accordance with the slight degree of menstrual activity. Specific Odor of Females during the Period of Heat. Many (but not all) males can evidently clearly distinguish between females which are in the period of heat on the one hand and those in which there is merely menstrual secretion and in SEXUAL CYCLE OF THE GUINEA PIG. 24! which both are lacking on the other hand. Therefore the male attempts copulation only in the former case. When a number of animals of both sexes are together and among them is a female in the heat period, the males run around and search for that particular female, ultimately finding it. I am convinced from this searching of the male for the female in heat that he is led to do so by a special odor characteristic of the female, when it reaches the heat period. This peculiarity is common to the mouse, the rat and the rabbit. Influence of Food upon the Character of Heat and on the Time when Sexual Maturity is Reached. The vigor of the heat in guinea pigs depends upon the nourish- ment which the animals have received. Guinea pigs which had been poorly fed develop weak heat. This applies to the duration of the heat as well as to the vigor of the symptoms. The length of the dicestral period does on the other hand not seem to be affected by the state of nourishment. I made observations on the influence of food on the time when sexual maturity is reached. I. On especially prepared rich food the first heat was observed 45 to 60 days after birth. II. On usual food 55 to 70 days after birth. III. On food of poor quality 75 to 100 days after birth. The usual food consisted of hay (or grass), carrots (or beets) and oats. In the rich diet corn cake was added, while the poor diet consisted merely of hay (or green grass) and oats. When the quality of the food was above the ordinary, its effect on the size of the guinea pig as well as their physical condition in other respects could be seen. They were of the average condition, when ordinary food was given at regular intervals, and the poorly fed animals were poorly developed physically. The food is therefore of the greatest importance as far as the time of sexual maturity and the character of the heat are concerned. Influence of Climate on the Character of the Heat and the Length of Sexual Cycle in the Guinea Pig. The extreme temperature of the summer months (a tempera- ture of over 90° F. in Granby, Mass.) has a weakening effect on 242 O. ISHII. the guinea pigs. And the heat is therefore not so vigorous as it would be under other climatic conditions. The length of the dioestrous period on the other hand seemed to be the same during the warm and cold season of the year. Does Contact with the Male Influence the Time of Appearance of the First Heat? If young female guinea pigs are kept in a cage with males and other guinea pigs of the same descent are kept separated from males, the first heat appears in both lots at the same age. This shows that psychical factors such as the stimulation of the male does not influence those conditions which lead to the first heat. The Copulation-reflex of the Heat Period and the Diagnosis of the Heat Energy. This reflex can be elicited by the male guinea pig seeking copulation, when it touches or gently massages with his forefeet the lumbar portion of the spinal column of the female which is in heat. The female will then lift up the tail and the vagina will be opened in preparation of the copulation. The same reflex can be called forth if an observer touches with his finger the same part of the body of the female guinea pig. The readiness and vigor with which the reflex occurs can serve as a measure of the energy or vigor of the heat. Menstruation during Pregnancy. I very carefully watched twenty guinea pigs during their state of pregnancy for a sign of menstruation and found in ten no evidence of menstrual secretion, but in the remaining ten there was some secretion present; not all of those however showed signs of heat. The menstrual period is not definite during pregnancy. Statistically my results were as follows: Menstruation was noticed in 3 guinea pigs 27 days after copulation " 3 " " 30 " " 2 " " 40 " 2 " " had menstrual secretion twice, the first time 15 days and the second time 30 days after copulation. While in these cases the swelling of the external genitalia and the secretions are manifest, they are slight. The secreted material SEXUAL CYCLE OF THE GUINEA PIG. 243 is mostly transparent, but sometimes turbid. It continued for 3 to 8 days in my observation; then the external genitalia returned to a normal condition. Some authors suppose that the diagnosis of the menstrual secretion is very difficult in the guinea pig; however careful observation has shown me that the diagnosis can be readily made and hardly misinterpreted. Diagnosis of Pregnancy about 14 to 15 Days after Copulation. About 14 or 15 days after copulation we can correctly determine the success or failure of the impregnation. If at that time the external genitalia are in a normal condition we may almost always assume a successful pregnancy. If, on the other hand, there should be within that time a noticeable swelling of the external genitalia, accompanied by secretion, this may in all cases be taken as an indication of failure. As we have stated above, the same phenomenon may be present, perhaps in a milder form, at this period of pregnancy, but this is a very rare occurrence; and as a general rule in the case of failure (early abortion) the secretion and swelling is much more marked. The Sexual Cycle in Cases of Early Abortion. In cases of early abortion I found the period of heat in some cases somewhat accelerated and more irregular than in normal animals. DlOESTRUM. Length of Heat Following Copulation. 1.2% Days. 13^ Days. 14% Days. 15^ Days. 16 Days. No. of guinea pigs examined I 2 2 -j 5 As shown in the table, the heat period may appear as early as 12^2 or J3^ days after copulation. In control cases the shortest period observed was 14^ days and this occurred only once; and, as I stated above, I believe that in normal cases 15 days represents the minimum and that the shorter periods are usually found in cases of abortion. 244 O. ISHII. The Length of the Period of Gestation in the Guinea Pig. On the basis of our observations we can give the following statistical data concerning the length of the period of pregnancy in the guinea pig. Length of Gestation Period. 61 Days. 62 Days. 64 Days. 66 Days. 67y2 Days. 68 Days. r> Days. Days. No. of pregnant guinea viss . 2 2 4 4 8 15 5 I The shortest period was 61 days; but those young animals which are born 61, 62 and 64 days following copulation show a certain lack of development; they are really born prematurely and in the majority of cases they soon die, although they may be able to survive. Those born 66 days after copulation usually lived and continued to grow, but those born from 67 to 69 days following copulation show the maximal development after birth ; these have the best chance to develop into absolutely healthy, perfect animals. We may therefore conclude that 68 days is the regular period of gestation in the guinea pig. Separation of the Symphysis Pubis in the Guinea Pig at the End of Pregnancy, at the Time of Birth and During the Period Directly Following Birth. Through examination with the finger we discern in the guinea pig a gradual separation of the symphysis pubis which begins about 6 1 to 63 days after copulation and increases as the time of labor approaches to a width of 7 to 8 millimeters or more; but within one or one and a half days after birth the symphysis pubis assumes again its normal condition. The following table shows the time when the separation becomes apparent : Beginning Separation of Symphysis Pubis. 61 Days, Following Copulation. 62 Days, Following Copulation. 63 Days, Following Copulation. No. of pregnant guinea pigs. 5 5 4 The length of time from the beginning of the separation of the symphysis to the onset of labor is shown on the following table: SEXUAL CYCLE OF THE GUINEA PIG. 245 Duration of Period of Separation of Symphysis Pubis Prior to Birth. 4% Days. 5 Days. 7 Days. No. of pregnant guinea pigs ' 6 7 The following table shows the length of time necessary for the symphysis pubis to return to its normal condition following labor : Time when the Symphysis Pubis has again Become Normal Following Labor. X Day. i Day. iyz Days. •zY2 Days. No. of guinea pigs 8 c; I From these data we may conclude that the separation of the symphysis pubis begins from 5 to -7 days previous to birth, or from the 6ist to the 63d day after copulation. Under ordinary conditions birth should not take place before the 5th day following this separation. The length of the period of gestation should therefore usually be 68 days. Preparation for Labor. In the guinea pig the separation of the symphysis pubis serves as a preparation for parturition, inasmuch as the orifice of the normal pelvis is too narrow to permit birth to take place. Moreover we find at the same time the same remarkable softness and elasticity of the muscles and joints as during the ordinary menstrual period. This same effect (emanating from the ovary or pituitary gland) I have also noticed in the rat and in the rabbit and I presume it is the same in all mammals. The Time of the First Heat in the Puerperium. The time of the beginning of heat in the period directly follow- ing labor is somewhat variable. In 20 guinea pigs in which parturition set in at the normal time (67 to 69 days after copula- tion) I found the following figures : Beginning Puerperal Heat After Parturition. Soon After 3 Hours After. 5 Hours After. 7 Hours After. No Heat. No. of examined guinea pigs 7 7 3 I 2 246 O. ISHII. As our table shows, in 2 out of 20 guinea pigs no heat at all was observed at this period. There was evidence pointing to the conclusion that the heat is less vigorous at this stage than in the, normal menstrual period. On the other hand the heat seems perhaps to be of a somewhat longer duration at this period as shown in the following table: Duration of Puerperal Heat. 7 to g Hrs. 10 to ii Hrs. 12 to 13 Hrs. No. of guinea nigs . , 8 7 7 We find therefore that in about one third of the guinea pigs at this period the average duration of heat was 12 to 13 hours, while in normal animals the heat continues for that length of time only in a small minority of cases. Whether or not heat occurs in the period directly following parturition depends upon the duration of the period of gestation as shown in the following table : Duration of Pregnancy. Efl M >, *o n a en N >* ^o rt Q t/j •«• >* >o a Q in MD >* ^o a P x£ f~ !8 VOQ tn 00 >* \o ri Q Ifl o >* ^o « Q n M >* ^ No. of observed guinea pigs. Puerperal heat . 2 — 2 2 — 2 4 — 4 4 -I/+3 o -I/+7 15 — 2/+I3 5 + S r + T + coming in heat; — not in heat. This table shows that heat does not occur in the period directly following parturition in cases in which the duration of pregnancy was less than 66 or 67 days. In cases in which the duration of pregnancy was less, birth occurred prematurely and the phe- nomena of the puerperium had not yet fully developed at the time of the completion of labor. If in cases of parturition occurring 61 or 62 days after copulation the first heat following parturition would take place 6 or 8 days later, the first heat would correspond to a typical puerperal heat, provided the duration of pregnancy would have been 68 or 69 instead of 61 days. SEXUAL CYCLE OF THE GUINEA PIG. 247 On the Duration of the Sexual Cycle following Parturition. The time of the second menstruation in the period following parturition is rather variable as shown in the following table: Development of Second Menstruation Following Parturition. ^£ m rt MQ tfl VO X M B p NT 1 « WX >> *o rt "P E/> t^X M « Q :«£ 1- B HP yi 00 >i >- rt P (fl H >* M Ctf P ui M >< "p ui m >% N rt P rt O H No. of guinea pigs 2 I 4 5 2 3 I I I 20 Among 20 guinea pigs in the period following the puerperium I found complete menstruation and heat in 15 cases, while in 5 animals I found only menstrual secretion without heat. The majority of these animals suckled their young and among those the deviation from normal was somewhat less; in these cases the duration was mostly 151/2 to 18 days. Effect of Lactation and Non-Lactation on the Periodicity of the Sexual Cycle after Parturition. In cases in which the young were suckled the next period of heat took place approximately at the normal time and the condi- tions continued from then on in a normal manner. It was dif- ferent in cases in which bodily weakness of the mother prevented the suckling of the young. In the latter cases the return of menstruation was delayed so that it occurred as late as 21 to 23 days after parturition. In the normal suckling animal the sexual periodicity following labor is therefore similar to that of the normal animal, although certain minor variations depending on the vitality of the animal occur even in such cases. The second or third menstruations occur usually after the regular interval of 15 to 16 days even in the weak animal. In the latter, hbweven we> may at these times instead of heat merely find menstrual secretion. The Effect of Suckling on the Mother and the Young. Marshall1 expresses the opinion that in the young separated prematurely from the mother growth will continue. My own observations lead me to a different conclusion. I have found 1 F. H. A. Marshall, "The Physiology of Reproduction." London, 1910. 248 O. ISHII. that if a mother suckled three young, the growth of the latter was very slow, or death resulted; in addition the weakening effect on the mother was quite noticeable; it sometimes caused her death, and in most cases the death of the young occurred. If the young died, the mother soon regained her health and strength. The result will therefore be disappointing either as far as the condition of the mother or of the young is concerned. It has been my experience that when the young were taken away from the mother within 15 or 20 days after birth and the ordinary vegetable and oat diet was given them, development was very slow and in most cases death occurred. In any case the young to attain healthy growth should not be taken from the mother until more than 25 to 30 days of age. Under natural conditions they cease suckling after 35 to 40 days, when they seek their own food. I am therefore of the opinion that a litter of three very seldom shows normal development when suckled by one mother. When I combined one mother with two and another with three young, each mother suckled on the average 2^/2 young. In such cases I obtained good results. While two or three young is the average size of a litter, I have known the number to vary from one to six; the latter number, however, only occurred once in my observation. When but one young is born, it is usually well developed and vigorous; but to be successfully raised it must be suckled for the usual period or at least for a time ap- proaching it. There is another symptom in addition to the bodily weakness, which the mother shows as the result of too intensive suckling: it consists in the changing of the color of the pupil of the eye which becomes pale or white. It may be associated with general exhaustion. Under what Condition is Copulation followed by Pregnancy? As we stated above the normal duration of heat is about 10 hours. If copulation took place within 3 or 4 hours after the beginning of heat, pregnancy followed only rarely. If the copula- tion took place at a later period of the heat, better results were obtained. According to my observations pregnancy occurred in about 73 per cent, to 80 per cent, of the cases. In such cases in which copulation was not followed by pregnancy a second or SEXUAL CYCLE OF THE GUINEA PIG. 249 third attempt during the next two periods of heat were mostly successful. Sobotta1 states that the guinea pig in captivity can become pregnant more frequently in summer than in winter. My observations do not bear out this statement. In my experience the pregnancies resulted almost as often in the cold as in the hot season. The -winter climate is better adapted for the guinea pig in captivity than the hot summer weather when the tempera- ture is liable to exceed 90° F. Under those conditions the animal suffers and becomes poorly developed as the result of under- nourishment. On the other hand, it is almost always in good condition in winter. Therefore the cold season agrees better with guinea pigs than the hot season under conditions of cap- tivity. In exceptional cases the guinea pig may become in winter time too fat to be favorable for impregnation; and this may even happen in summer time. Relation between Growth, the First Period of Heat and the Period of Sexual Maturity in the Guinea Pig. In my observations the weight of the guinea pig is ordinarily three ounces soon after birth. However, the weight of the young is in accordance with the age of the mother and also the number of the litter. As a rule the young born from a young mother are small in size and in litter. On the other hand, when the mother is older than 5 months, the weight of the young is mostly larger. For instance, in one litter the young weighed 6 or 7 ounces soon after birth. Minot2 has concluded that the guinea pig is slow in growing and that it attains its full size one year after birth. My observa- tions are as follows: The young guinea pig at the time of the first heat was about two months old; about 7 to 10 days before the beginning of heat, . the growth almost stopped or at least progressed very slowly. At the time of the first heat period the average weight was n to 13 ounces. After the first heat period had passed it began to grow very rapidly. About two or three 1 Sobotta, Anal. Hefte, 1906, XXXII. 2 Minot, C. S., "Growth and Senescence," Journ. Physiol., 1891, XII., 97. "Problems of Age, Growth and Death," Popular Science Monthly, 1907, LXX.. 481, and LXXI., 97 and succeeding numbers. 250 O. ISHII. months later, at the age of 4 to 5 months, it attained its full weight, about 20 to 28 ounces, but one exceptional animal weighed 34 ounces in a non-pregnant condition. However, if we feed a special diet of nutrient material and the animal is otherwise well taken care of, then the growth progresses rapidly and the heat begins earlier than usual. The guinea pig which weighed 34 ounces was fed on the usual farm or laboratory food. Therefore we must remember that the growth of the young depends considerably upon the character of the food. Vol. XXXVIII. May, 1920. No. 5. BIOLOGICAL BULLETIN SELECTIVE FERTILIZATION IN POLLEN MIXTURES. D. F. JONES, CONNECTICUT AGRICULTURAL EXPERIMENT STATION, NEW HAVEN. Equality in fertilizing power of gametes dissimilar in the hereditary factors they carry is a corollary of Mendelism. It has been thought that certain exceptions to this general rule may exist. Differential fecundating ability has been suggested from time to time as a possible interpretation of otherwise un- accountable results. The earlier uses of this hypothesis of inequality in sperm efficiency, such as Castle's (1903) theory of sex determination, Cuenot's (1908) conception of the reason for the non-appearance of homozygous yellow mice, have since been found to be unnecessary. The interpretation of linkage phe- nomena according to the reduplication hypothesis, while essen- tially different in application, holds somewhat the same implica- tion as selective fertilization, that is, differential operative power of cells of unlike germinal construction. As the yellow mouse problem was finally resolved to a selective elimination of zygotes so many puzzles have had considerable light thrown on them by a more complete understanding of the factor relations. Many instances of the non-appearance of cer- tain types are known to be the result of the action of lethal factors. Good illustrations of/ this are found in Drosophila (Muller, 1918). Other cases of elimination immediately after fertilization or early in development are known in maize. Sometimes abortion of a part of the gametes before fertilization takes place and it is assumed that the elimination is selective. Belling (1914) has shown that in Stizolobium 50 per cent, of the pollen grains and ovules regularly abort in certain types. The aberrant results from the (Enotheras are now generally considered largely, if not wholly, to be due to differential destruction of both 251 252 D. F. JONES. gametes and zygotes. Davis (1915-' 17) has called attention to the great amount of pollen and ovule abortion in these plants and to the low germination of the seeds which are produced and to the results which are obtained when a more complete germination is secured. It is not always easy to distinguish between differential destruc- tion of zygotes immediately after fertilization and selective fertilization. It is still more difficult in the case of abortion of germ cells. In fact it is only a matter of degree between selective elimination of gametes and selective fertilizing ability- From germ cells which are unable to complete development on account of the particular inheritances they carry, to gametes which appear normal but are unable to function perfectly under any circumstances, it is only a step. However, no instance of the latter condition is positively known and the cases of gametic abortion clearly due to the factors which the gametophytes carry are rare. Although results still remain to be cleared up which indicate a selective action of some kind (Kempton, 1919) the general con- clusion holds, which is, that the pollen, although gametophytic itself, has the function of the sporophyte which produces it. This is supported by the well-known cases where pollen color and shape are all of the type of the maternal parent irrespective of the factors for pollen characters which they carry. Many instances are known where gametes, containing lethal factors which stop development immediately after fertilization, are able to function fully as well as others permitting normal growth. East and Park (1917) have shown that compatibility in Nicotiana is alike for all the gametes of any one individual. Different pollen grains may carry factors which will determine sterility or fertility of the future zygotes but all function alike in fertiliza- tion according to the sporophyte from which they come. Follow- ing up these results East (1919) shows that in the frequency dis- tributions of pollen tube lengths there is no significant difference between pollen from plants greatly unlike in the degree of heterozogosity. In other words gametes carrying markedly di- verse germinal complexes are no more variable than those all of like constitution in ability to send pollen tubes down the styles SELECTIVE FERTILIZATION IN POLLEN MIXTURES. 253 and to accomplish fertilization. East considers that chance segregation in the germ cells and random mating of these germ cells is a fundamental genetic hypothesis applicable to plants and animals alike. With this conclusion the writer is in accord, with the reservation that the evidence from gametic abortion should not be put aside as belonging to a different category. The data to be presented here bear upon another phase of the problem. The above conclusion, it should be clearly kept in mind, applies only to gametes produced by one or more indi- viduals of the same type. That is, the gametes may be unlike in the factors they carry, but if they come from the same or similar individuals they are potentially equal in ability to fertilize. But what is the result when germ cells from two individuals of different type are presented at the same time in excess so that not all can fulfill their function? Will fertilization take place at random or not? This is the problem to be con- sidered here. In the one case doses of different kinds of medicine come in the same capsules. In the other the capsules as well as their contents may differ. Anyone accustomed to swallowing a particular kind of capsule made of a familiar substance and all of the same size and shape can take any kind of medicine with equal readiness irrespective of the result which will ensue when the materials within the containers begin to operate. Applying this crude metaphor to plants and animals, in the one case, the germ cells, however they differ in factorial composition, come in the same cytoplasmic envelope; in the other, the cytoplasm as well as the genes may differ. It has already been shown that, in particular cases, there is a selective action when pollen from different plants is applied to a stigma at the same time. East (1919) has demonstrated that in a mixture of compatible and -incompatible pollen placed on the stigmas of self-sterile Nicotianas the compatible pollen alone functions. Two experiments were devised to test this. Small numbers of pollen grains were counted out under a microscope and placed on stigmas known to be receptive to that kind of pollen. The stigmas were then covered with a large amount of incompatible pollen. The former only was able to fertilize as shown by the results from eight such mixed pollinations which 254 D. F. JONES. in no case produced a greater number of seeds than the number of compatible pollen grains applied. In the other experiment a constant white-flowered self-sterile plant was pollinated with its own pollen. Several hours later pollen of a red-flowered plant was applied. Abundant seed was secured and when grown red-flowered cross-fertilized plants only were found. In both these cases there was complete selective fertilization in favor of the pollen from dissimilar plants. According to the results to be reported here a directly reversed effect is obtained from mixed pollinations in maize. In many trials the plant's own pollen has been more efficient in accom- plishing fertilization than that from other individuals which differ only in minor features. This same pollen which is less effective when in competition with the plant's own kind of pollen is fully able to function when not applied in mixtures. The results are remarkable in view of the notable advantages which cross-fertilization gives to the immediately resulting seeds and the plants grown from them. The material used consisted largely of self-fertilized strains which had been brought to uni- formity and constancy and were considerably reduced in size and vigor. In this material crossing is known to increase the weight of seed within the same inflorescences as much as 50 per cent, in some cases. This greater amount of material is laid down both in the embryo and endosperm, and is apparent in the greater size of the seeds which show a higher specific gravity together with more rapid maturation indicated by their lower water content at the close of the growing period. A large series of mixed pollinations show the ability of the cross-fertilized seeds to germinate better by an average of 16 per cent. The resulting plants start to grow sooner, develop faster, mature in a shorter time and at the end far surpass their self-pollinated sibs. The amount of heterosis shown by maize is possibly greater than that displayed by any other plants in intra-specific crosses. Production of grain has been advanced on an average of 180 per cent., height of plant 27, length of pistillate spike 29, number of rows of spikelets 5 and number of nodes 6 per cent, in a study of a large number of crosses between inbred strains (Jones, 1918). In spite of these great immediate advantages to be secured the SELECTIVE FERTILIZATION IN POLLEN MIXTURES. 255 plants manifest a decided preference for their own kind of pollen. This is a result which would have surprised students of flower pollination in Darwin's time but which, as I shall attempt to point out later, is in agreement with other results from biological • investigation. METHODS OF CARRYING ON THE EXPERIMENTS. In an investigation in which it was desired to compare the chemical composition of seeds of maize having different genetic constitution but produced under as nearly identical conditions as possible advantage was taken of endosperm characters to enable proper classification of seeds produced in the same inflores- cences. For example, two kinds of pollen carrying yellow endo- sperm color and white endosperm color were mixed together and applied to a plant which normally produces uncolored seeds. The resulting yellow and white seeds were distributed at random on the pistillate spikes and were as nearly comparable in respect to external and nutritional factors as it is possible to obtain. It was found that such pollen mixtures could be applied to both strains furnishing the pollen and the two kinds of seeds easily distinguished. On the white-seeded strain the seeds resulting from the plant's own pollen were white and the cross-fertilized seeds yellow. On the yellow-seeded plants the self-pollinated endosperms were dark yellow while the crossed seeds were dis- tinctly lighter in color, in most cases they had a white cap, and were as a rule readily separable. Attention is directed to the fact that the material used for these mixed pollinations consisted largely of inbred strains which had been reduced to uniformity and constancy so that the genetical differences between the two kinds of seeds sharply differentiated them, much more than in ordinary cross-pollinated varieties of this plant in which the yellow color is usually variable due to more than one factor for this color and various modifying conditions such as the consistency of the endosperm in respect to corneous and floury starch. A number of pairs of plants were treated in this way by mixing their pollen and applying to both types. After harvesting it was realized that here was an excellent method of determining 256 D. F. JONES. whether or not any selective action was shown by the plant's own pollen as compared to that from a plant of somewhat different type. If one member of the pair of plants which furnished the pollen for the mixture is designated A and the other B, the two kinds of seeds grown on A plants are A X A, self- fertilized, and A X B, cross-fertilized; on the B plants B X A cross-fertilized and B X B self- fertilized. Since the same pollen mixture is applied to both, the ratio of the seeds resulting from A pollen to the seeds resulting from B pollen, on A plants, should be the same as the ratio of the seeds resulting from A and from B pollen on B plants. In other words, the numbers form a proportion which, irrespective of the relative amounts of A and B pollen in the mixture, should be a perfect proportion within the limits of random sampling if fertilization takes place at random. The end terms of the proportion comprise the self-pollinated seeds and the middle terms the reciprocally cross- pollinated seeds. If a true proportion is obtained the products of the end terms should naturally equal the products of the middle terms. If they do not the deviation is either in an excess of cross-fertilized or of self-fertilized seeds indicating a selective action in one or the other direction. The advantages of this method of attacking the problem are readily apparent. It is practically impossible to make up a mixture of large amounts of pollen in which the proportion of each is known. Either measuring or weighing the pollen is out of the question because maize pollen takes up moisture from the air rapidly and when any quantity is brought together it becomes aggregated into a flocculent mass. Moreover this pollen soon loses its viability so that even in case equal numbers of pollen grains could be had there would be no proof that there were equal numbers of functional pollen grains in the mixture. The method of reciprocal application and arranging the results in the form of a proportion automatically overcomes all these difficulties and the experiment is as simple as could be devised. An attempt was made to have as nearly equal quantities of pollen as possible by measuring out the two kinds roughly. But in many cases the results showed that one kind of pollen was far more effective than the other. This, however, does not destroy SELECTIVE FERTILIZATION IN POLLEN MIXTURES. 257 the value of the figures as it is the relative efficiency of each type of pollen, when it is applied to its own and to foreign stigmas, that is being investigated. In the preliminary experiments there were 18 pairs of plants which produced seed as the result of the application of mixed pollen. When these were counted and the deviations of the proportions found from the closest perfect proportions were obtained there were 12 mixtures which showed a deviation in favor of the self-fertilized seeds and 6 in the opposite direction. The results as a whole showed a tendency to favor the plant's own pollen. This was somewhat unexpected so that it was con- sidered worth while to investigate the matter more fully. The inbred material was so reduced in growth that the number of seeds produced on one plant was not large enough to give the results much weight. It was therefore decided to pollinate a number of plants of two different self-fertilized strains with the same mixture. Most of the strains used had been self-pollinated for six generations or more, some as many as ten, so that the plants within one strain were practically identical in hereditary constitution. Pollen was collected from about the same number of plants as the pollen was applied to. The two lots of pollen were put together in a paper sack and thoroughly mixed by shaking. This mixture was then applied to plants of the two strains which supplied the pollen. It was desired to have from ten to fifteen plants in each of the paired strains so as to give from 1,000 to 2,000 seeds in each of the two parts of the propor- tions but the flowers were not always ready at the right time and some pollinations were failures for a variety of reasons so that not as large numbers as desired were secured in every case. Every effort was made to prevent the entrance of undesired pollen. The technique has been described and the amount of experimental error due to contamination to be expected in artificial pollination of maize has been considered previously (East and Hayes, 1911). In the course of these experiments the effects of extraneous pollen were seen in very small numbers compared to the total number. This source of error could be detected in the seeds when colors or other characters differing from either of the strains used were brought in by the undesired 258 D. F. JONES. pollen. In a total of 63,000 seeds produced in these experiments only about 30 illegitimate seeds were observed. A larger number of contaminations, however, would have the same appearance as the legitimate seeds and so could not be detected but giving a reasonable allowance to this source of unreliability the results could not be appreciably altered. Moreover the error of this kind is never all in one direction so that in the main it can be disregarded. The accuracy with which the seeds have been classified de- serves particular attention. Strains were selected to be used which gave sharp differences between self-fertilized and cross- fertilized seeds and in most cases separation was made very satisfactorily. In a few mixtures there was some doubt and in two experiments the seeds on the yellow endosperm plants could not be distinguished. In these two cases the seeds were planted and classification was made with the mature plants. Also in all the other mixtures involving yellow and white endosperms a sample was taken, after all the seeds of one class were mixed together, and grown to determine the per cent, of error in sepa- rating the seeds. Since the self-fertilized seeds give small inbred plants, pure for yellow or white color, while the cross-fertilized seeds produce large vigorous hybrids segregating into yellow and white seeds, classification of the mature plants can be made without the least doubt. However, it should be noted that the better germination and greater vigor give the advantage to the cross-fertilized classes in every case if there is any difference. About 1 20 plants in each lot were grown and the per cent, of error obtained was used to calculate the total amount of error if all the seeds had been grown. Since the numbers of seed ran up into the thousands it was impossible to grow all of them. The figures showing the per cent, of errors found are arranged in Table III. Only in one case is the number of wrongly classified individuals above 3 per cent. In 25 out of 44 lots no faulty separations were discovered. In the remaining cases the mis- placed seeds tend to balance each other so that this source of doubt can be largely removed. Calculating the figures without regard to the error of classification gives practically the same results as when this is taken into consideration. SELECTIVE FERTILIZATION IN POLLEN MIXTURES. 259 Different characters were used in other mixed pollinations which permitted even more positive classification than the single character difference of yellow and white endosperms. Plants with yellow sweet seeds were paired with white starchy plants. Each strain brought in a dominant character. White starchy plants of the pop or Zea mays everta type were used because the clear corneous endosperm differentiates very clearly between yellow and white. In the reciprocal cross the smooth opaque seeds show up plainly contrasted with the wrinkled translucent seeds. Similarly purple sweet and white starchy types were paired. In a few cases the purple crossed seeds were not as distinct as could be desired, but the error is certainly small. These latter mixtures were made the past season and no plants have been grown to test the reliability of their separation but the writer is confident that the number of wrongly classified seeds, if any, is not sufficient to alter the results, appreciably. In many inflorescences a few seeds were found which had failed to reach a stage of development so that they could be classified. Seeds at the tips of the spikes and where the seeds were closely crowded were abortive. This introduces another source of unreliability, that of selective elimination of zygotes. Since crossing gives to seeds of maize an enormous advantage in development, it can confidently be expected that as a rule more of the self-pollinated seeds will be found among the abortions than cross-pollinated. What we are attempting to study is the •relative fertilizing efficiency of different types of pollen grains. But one can only arrive at this by counting the zygotes sometime after fertilization has taken place. In the meantime a differential destruction of zygotes may have taken place. This effect must be considered in any organisms employed but because of the short time which elapses between fertilization and the maturation of the seeds, and from the fact that they develop in an exceedingly favorable and uniform environment, maize is the very best material the writer can think of in which this problem can be attacked, especially when the numbers which can be obtained are taken into consideration. All animals and those plants which d3 not show zenia in the seeds have the objection that a comparatively long time elapses between fertilization and suffi- 26o D. F. JONES. cient maturity to permit classification. In plants many cases are known in which there is a selective elimination of certain classes of individuals due to a lesser germination and unequal ability to grow. In Drosophila (Hyde, 1914) crossing does not influence the number of eggs laid but markedly regulates the per cent, that hatch. Therefore the error from this source always tends to show an apparent deviation in favor of cross- fertilization. In maize where the seed progenies can be used this differentiating effect is at a minimum and probably is not suffi- cient to affect the numbers appreciably but since the tendency is in the opposite direction to the results which have been obtained the data are even more convincing. TABLE I. CHARACTERS OF THE SEEDS AND PEDIGREE NUMBERS OF THE INBRED STRAINS AND FIRST HYBRIDS USED IN THE MIXED POLLINATIONS. GENERATION Pollen Mixture Number. Characters of the Seeds of Pedigree Number of A Strain. B. Strain. A Strain. E. Strain. I Yellow Starchy ti ( t a « 4 ( I ( It ( ( ft tt Yellow Sweet Yellow and White Sweet Purple Sweet t < * ( n n 1 1 it White Starchy ti u it it It It 14 tl tl It tl It I ( tt tt 41 41 tt tl I t White Starchy tt it tt it tt tt White Starchy tt it 14 tt " 1-9-1-2-4-6-7-5-6-2-1 1-9-1-2-4-6-7-5-3-2-1 14-4-6-16-2-12-22 1 4-4—6—16—2—1 2-2 2 I4-4-6-I6—2—I2-22 I4-30-4-3-7-II-IO I4-30-4-3-7-II-IO I4-4-6-I6-2-I2-22 I-9-I-2-4-6-7-5-6-2-I-I (1-6-1-3 X 1-9-1-2) Fi (1-6-1-3 X 1-7-1-1) Fi (1-7-1-2 X 1-6-1-3) Fi 146-1-1 146-1-1 (126-1-1-1 X 77) Fi (126-1-1-1 X 77) Fi 76-2-2-1-1 76-2-1-2-1 76-2-1-2-1 76-2-2-1-1 10-4-8-3-5-3-4-8-2-1 I 0-4-8-3-5-3-4-5-2-1 21-13-9-7-57-11 21-13-9-7-57-11 21-13-9-7-57-11 2I-I3-9-7-57-I I 20-4-25-47-4 20-8-5-35-20 20-4-25-47-4 (20-8 X 21-13) Fi (20-8 X 21-13) Fi (21-13 X 20-8) Fi 65-8-2-2-6-5-3-2-1-1-1 65-8 -2-2-6-5-3-2-1-1-1 117-3-1-1 117-1-1-1 117—1—1—1 117-1-1-1 20-4-25-47-24-1 20-4-25-47-24-1 2 3 4 ? 6 7 8 Q ... IO II 12 I"? 14 15 16 17 18 IO. . 20. . . i C Strain C Strain Yellow Starchy White Sweet White Starchy 1-6-1-3-4-4-4-2-4-4-2-5 77-2-1-1-1 117-1-1-1 19 20 SELECTIVE FERTILIZATION IN POLLEN MIXTURES. 26l MATERIALS USED AND PRESENTATION OF THE RESULTS. The figures obtained from the preliminary experiments in which pairs of single plants only were used are not given here because the numbers are too low to give the results much value and for fear of making this report too bulky. It is sufficient to remember that the data taken together indicated a slight selec- tive action favoring the plant's own pollen. TABLE II. SUMMARY OF ALL THE POLLEN MIXTURES GIV.ING THE TOTAL NUMBER OF SEEDS, THE NUMBERS IN EACH CLASS FORMING PROPORTIONS, THE DEVIATIONS OF WHICH, EXPRESSED AS PER CENT., FROM THE CLOSEST TRUE PROPORTIONS, ARE ALMOST WHOLLY IN FAVOR OF THE PLANT'S OWN POLLEN. P is THE PROB- ABILITY THAT THESE DEVIATIONS ARE DUE TO THE DIFFERENCES OF RANDOM SAMPLING. Pollen Total Mixture Number Number, j of Seeds. AX A. AXB. BXA. BXB. Deviation from True Proportion, Per Cent. X2- P. I 2 3,430 5,636 1,738 2,133 46 145 1,602 3.080 44 278 + -045 + -955 .027 7-572 -994 .063 3 4 5 6 1,362 3,344 1,956 424 229 710 589 40 14 126 6 7i 770 1,856 1,290 187 349 652 7i 126 + 12.715 + 5-465 + 2.105 -11.850 146.196 61.203 28.718 23-858 .000 .000 .000 .000 7 8 3-459 7,783 23 2,185 89 956 1,507 2,619 1,840 2,023 -12.245 + 6.570 235-357 144.108 .000 .000 9 8,729 2,550 1,288 2,922 1,969 + 3-350 42.061 ; .000 10 5,408 ii 3-314 12 3-561 1,084 448 1,724 I-I54 264 719 997 1.505 749 2,173 1,097 369 + 8.495 + 2.540 + 1-790 162.687 8-937 5.313 .000 -030 •053 13 14 736 792 185 424 39i 150 95 156 65 62 -13-625 + I-I55 55-051 •533 .000 .894 15 16 3,i68 2,224 2,609 723 47 8 14 74 498 1,419 +47-750 +46.975 2,889.561 1,965.981 .000 .000 17 1,410 1,303 18 1,599 4 3 21 4 i 100 1,573 +47-960 + 7-970 1,298.993 I37-532 .000 .000 19 20 2,606 2,753 528 897 392 77 343 1,174 1.343 605 + 18.525 + 13-050 376.394 283.002 .000 .000 63,694 Of those experiments in which pollen was applied to several plants of the same type 20 pollen mixtures in all have been made. 262 D. F. JONES. Each mixture is given a number and the seed characters of the materials used and their pedigree numbers are given in Table I. The first number in the pedigree designates the variety from which the inbred strains were derived. The following numbers indicate the progenitors in the successive self-fertilized genera- tions. The total number of units in the pedigree number less one, show the number of generations the material had been self-fertilized at the time the pollinations were made. The inbred strains used in these experiments are as follows : i. Several distinct strains from a yellow dent variety originally obtained in Illinois and known as Chester's Learning, self- fertilized ten or more years. 10. A strain with white floury seeds with no traces of corneous starch, self-fertilized nine generations. 14. Two distinct strains from a yellow dent variety from Con- necticut known as Stadtmueller's Learning, selected for high protein content during six generations of self-fertilization. 20. Two distinct strains from a white dent variety originally selected for high protein at the Illinois Exper. Station and further selected during four generations of self-fertilization. 21. One strain from same source as above but selected for low protein in field pollinated cultures and during five generations of self-fertilization. 65. A small, white, round-seeded strain from a variety of pop- corn, Zea mays everta, self-fertilized ten times and characterized by clear corneous starch. 76. Two similar strains from a sweet variety of latent flint type having purple aleurone and known as Black Mexican, self-fertilized four years. 77. From a sweet variety of latent dent type with deeply wrinkled white seeds known as Evergreen and self-fertilized four generations. 126. From a small, early maturing, yellow, sweet variety of latent flint type with dark yellow kernels, known as Golden Bantam, and self-fertilized three generations. 117. From a variety of popcorn with sharp pointed seeds having clear corneous endosperm, self-fertilized three times. 146. From a yellow, sweet variety, Golden Bantam, of different SELECTIVE FERTILIZATION IN POLLEN MIXTURES. 263 source than 126, and somewhat different in type, self-fertilized two years. Mixtures number i to 9 inclusive comprise various inbred strains with yellow starchy and white starchy endosperm. Some of these strains have been described previously (Jones, 1918) and all show marked heterosis in the crossed seeds and in the resulting first generation hybrid plants. A sample of all the different lots of seed secured in these mixtures have been grown to test the accuracy of classification. Pollen mixtures number 10 to 12 are not from inbred strains but from first generation hybrids, one having all yellow seeds the other all white. They were of such a constitution that the second crossing gave still more increase in vigor although not as great as the stimulus following the first cross. The plants being vigorous a large amount of seed was obtained from a few plants. It was desired to know whether the same selective action would be shown by vigorous plants with segregating gametes as contrasted with non-vigorous plants whose gametes were all alike. Classification of the seed was easily carried out and the per cent, of error when tested was found to be quite low. Mixtures 13 and 14 involved yellow sweet in one strain and white starchy endosperm in the other. One dominant factor was carried by each so that differentiation was perfectly distinct in the reciprocal applications. In mixtures 15 and 16 it was intended to make use of the same characters as in the two preceding numbers. The plants which were sup- posed to be inbred individuals of a yellow sweet strain, and so labeled, when grown in the field were seen to be too vigorous for this material as it had behaved in previous years. The plants were all alike, however, in this respect. It was suspected that this was a lot of first generation hybrids instead of plants from self-fertilized seed. As some crosses had been made with plants of this line the previous season it is now certain that in this instance the seed was not properly labeled at harvest and so was planted for self-fertilized seed when in reality it was all cross- fertilized. As no other plants were available at the time they were needed these were used. Several self-pollinations were made at the same time to show what the seed characters were of this undoubted hybrid. At maturity it definitely showed itself 264 D. F. JONES. to be a hybrid as all the ears were segregating for yellow and white sweet seeds. It therefore had certainly been crossed with a white sweet inbred strain the year before as other pollinations of this sort were made at that time. The yellow color of the endosperm was not sufficiently diluted to cause the seed to be suspicioned before planting. Since the plants were segregating for yellow and white it was not material which would have been used ordinarily for mixing with a white starchy strain. The effects of the starchy-carrying pollen showed up all right among the all sweet seeds but the reciprocal cross-pollination showed only the yellow cross-fertilized seeds. The white cross-fertilized seeds of course could not be distinguished from the self-fertilized seeds. But since half of the pollen grains carried yellow and half of them white the number of yellow seeds can be doubled to give the total number of cross- pollinated seeds and the assumed number of white cross-fertilized seeds subtracted from the white seeds. This increases the error from random sampling somewhat but since the number of yellow seeds is very low in comparison with the white in these mixtures, the data are reliable in view of the great selective action shown in these two mixtures. The fact that the yellow color in this material was a unit factor difference and that there were equal number of pollen grains carrying yellow and white is proven by the self-fertilized ears produced by the hybrid which gave very good 3 to i ratios (one ear counted gave 318 yellow and 97 white seeds). Furthermore the starchy crossed seeds produced on the hybrid plants were of two kinds, yellow and white, and were produced in equal numbers (actual numbers: 28 yellow and 27 white. Since the ovules were segregating equally and the self- fertilized seeds gave the mono-hybrid ratio the pollen grains must have carried the two colors in equal amount. In the last four mixtures the characters, purple sweet and white starchy, were used. In numbers 17 and 18 the plants were not producitve and the numbers of seeds are low. Also the classifica- tion of purple starchy cross-fertilized seeds and white starchy self-fertilized was not as sure as in the other mixtures. In the last two mixtures satisfactory numbers were obtained and the differentiation was clear-cut on both sides. SELECTIVE FERTILIZATION IN POLLEN MIXTURES. 265 The pollen mixtures I, 19 and 20 were also applied to a third strain distinct from either of the two used in supplying the pollen. The resulting two lots of seed in each case were both cross-fertilized and probably showed hybrid vigor in the seed in about equal amount. Heterosis is clearly apparent in the plants of similar crosses involving the same or closely related material and is approximately equal. The data from each pollen mixture are presented in the form of an appendix. Since the arrangement is the same in all the tables these are given in the simplest form possible and a descrip- tion of one applies to all. In the tables the headings for the four different classes of seed are A X A, A X B, B X A, B X B and in the three mixtures I, 19 and 20 there are in addition the out-crossed seeds C X A, and C X B. In every case the pistil- late parent is given first. These headings when expanded in detail are as follows : A X A = Self-fertilized seeds from color-carrying pollen. A X B == Cross " non-color " B X A == Cross " color B X B = Self " non-color " C X A == Out-crossed " color C X A == Out " non-color" On every plant there are two kinds of seeds indicating the relative fertilizing efficiency of the two kinds of pollen in the mixture used. The number of seeds from individual plants are given in the tables under their respective headings, A X A and A X B seed from the same inflorescence and B X A and B X B seeds from another and C X A and C X B from a third. The total number of seeds is summed up below the line. Next to that is given the per cent, of error found upon testing the accuracy of the classification. This figure is used to calculate the amount of error based upon all the seeds obtained. Not more than about 125 seeds were grown in each lot except in two cases as shown in Table III. The calculated amount of wrongly classified seeds is given on the line below the per cent, of error, then follows the corrected numbers after the proper additions and subtractions have been made. After that is placed the observed proportion 266 D. F. JONES. of the two kinds of seed in each member of the pair stated as per cent. The closest perfect proportion is calculated from this, based on the results from the A and B plants but not from the C plants, and this subtracted from the actual proportion found gives the deviation in per cent, which appears in the last line. This description applies to mixtures I to 12 inclusive. Of the remainder no correction for misplaced seeds is made. TABLE III. NUMBER OF PLANTS GROWN AND THE PER CENT. OF WRONGLY CLASSIFIED SEEDS IN THE POLLEN MIXTURES INVOLVING YELLOW AND WHITE, STARCHY ENDO- SPERM. Pollen A.xA. A. > T> B.) which by use of convenient tables calculated by Elderton, gives a probability value varying from o to I proportional to the goodness of fit. The calculations must be based on the actual numbers of seeds obtained and not on the percentages. To obtain a perfect pro- portion from which the deviations of the numbers found will be the smallest in the four terms it is necessary to balance the figures so that the same number of individuals are represented on the A and B plants. This is done by reducing the number of seeds of the greater and increasing the lesser keeping the ratios the same, of course. The deviations of the proportions, balanced in this way, from the closest perfect proportions are then used to obtain the probability value in the way described above. The same result is obtained more quickly by calculating the value of X2 from the percentages and multiplying this figure by one half the total number of seeds. SELECTIVE FERTILIZATION IN POLLEN MIXTURES. 269 With four terms the values of x2 greater than 18 have no probability out to three decimal places. Since very much greater numbers were secured in most cases, as shown in Table II., the deviations are clearly not due to the differences of random sampling alone if the application of this method of calculation is justifiable. The writer is not perfectly sure that it is because it should be noted that the theoretical proportion is calculated directly from the results found, that is, there is no possible way of knowing the real amounts of the two kinds of functional pollen contained in the mixtures. Moreover the probable error does not take into consideration the corrections which are made for the mistakes of classification found in a sample drawn from each lot. There may be a selective action when the pollen is applied to one plant but not to the other or the action may be reversed. All that is measured is the combined effect if both are in the same direction or the excess of one over the other if in opposite directions. It seems reasonable to suppose, however, that the selective fertilization is approximately the same on both members of the pair as a large number of mixed pollinations are avai able made with many different types of plants and the majority give the same result. If this were always true, however, the ratio obtained by out-crossing the mixture onto a distinct strain should not deviate from the ratio of the closest perfect proportion calculated from the figures of the reciprocal crosses beyond allow- able limits. That is, the ratio obtained from the out-crossed seeds is supposed to represent very nearly the actual ratio of effective pollen in the mixture since both kinds are more nearly on the same footing. In the three experiments, Nos. I, 19 and 20 in which such out-crossed seeds were obtained, the deviation is even greater in two cases than that from the reciprocal applica- tions. In pollen mixture number I all the deviations are small and probably without significance. Since the results are convincing when considered without a probable error, it is not necessary to lay much stress on the method of its calculation at this time. Considering the data altogether, magnifying the actual experimental error to its fullest extent, and taking a common-sense view of the allowances to be 270 D. F. JONES. made for variations inherent in a problem of this kind the con- clusion can be no other than that these plants manifest a definite receptiveness to their own pollen, discriminating against foreign pollen even though it comes from plants only slightly differen- tiated from them, both of which might easily be descended from the same individual at no very distant period back. This selec- tive action is shown by plants of weak growth or full vigor, whether each strain descended from a line of similar ancestors or whether its immediate parents were diverse and, finally, irrespective of the gametes being alike or unlike in germinal contents. The one significant feature in common in all these experiments is the fact that the cytoplasm which surrounds the male nuclei and which makes up the vehicle that carries them to the egg cells is alike for the gametes of any one type of plant whether this plant is homozygous or heterozygous and in self- fertilization this cytoplasm is the same as the medium in which the pollen fulfills its function. This points very strongly to the probability that the differential effect is due to the rate of pollen-tube growth although it may be determined after the male nuclei are brought to the egg. The average weight of the seeds in the different classes of all the pollen mixtures is given in Table IV. with the increase in weight of the cross-fertilized over the self-fertilized seeds. Expressed as per cent, these figures permit an estimation of the comparative amount of heterosis shown in the crosses. In fact this is one of the best means of measuring the stimulation of heterozygosis as the environmental differences are reduced to a minimum. It has not been definitely proven that there exists a correlation between the amount of heterosis in the seeds and that shown by the resulting plants grown from those seeds but the indications are that there is a close relation between the two. Since hybrid vigor is roughly proportional to the germinal differences in the two forms united it can be determined whether or not there is a relation between the diversity of the plants used in the several pollen mixtures and the degree of preference shown by those plants to their own kind of pollen. Table V. shows that the coefficient of correlation between the average increase in weight of seeds and the deviation in favor of self-fertilization, both stated SELECTIVE FERTILIZATION IN POLLEN MIXTURES. 271 TABLE IV. THE AMOUNT OF HETEROSIS SHOWN BY EACH CROSS-POLLINATION IN THE INCREASE IN AVERAGE WEIGHT OF SEEDS COMPARED TO THE SELF-POLLINATED SEEDS GROWN IN THE SAME INFLORESCENCES. Pollen Mixture Number. Average Weight of Seeds in Centigrams. Per Cent. Increase. AX A. AXB. Increase. BxA. BXB. Increase. A. B. Ave. I 2 13-8 12.2 18.1 17.1 4-3 4-9 18.0 17.6 15-7 14-3 2-3 3-3 30-4 40.0 14.6 23-0 22.5 31-5 3 4 5 6 29-3 26.9 30.0 35-5 31-4 47-61 6.2 4-5 17.6 39-8 34-2 38-3 36.7 35.7 29-3 34-8 34-7 4.1 4.9 3-5 2.O 21.2 I6.7 58-7 ii. 5 16.7 IO.I 5-8 16.4 I6.7 34-4 5-8 7 8 25-8 30.6 4.8 19.6 23-8 16.3 22.5 3-3 1-3 18.6 2O.2 5-8 20. 2 12.2 9 12. 0 15-8 3-8 15-7 13.2 2-5 31.7 18.9 25-3 10 ii 32.2 27-5 33-4 28.6 1.2 I.I 32.5 34-5 30-9 32.8 1.6 1.7 3-7 5-2 4.0 5.2 4-5 4.6 12 32-4 32.8 •4 37-4 34-3 3.1 1.2 9.0 5-i 13 14 25-7 20.2 29.3 22.1 3-6 1.9 16.4 13-9 14.8 12.9 1.6 I.O 14.0 9.4 10.8 7.8 12.4 8.6 IS 16 24.2 32.0 35-1 40.0' 10.9 8.0 12.9 11.4 ii. 7 9.6 1.2 1.8 45.0 25.0 10.3 18.8 27.7 21.9 17 18 14-3 25.0 20.0 23.81 5-7 — 1.2 15.0 10. 01 10.2 8.2 4.8 1.8 39-9 47-1 — 4.8 22.0 43-5 8.6 19 20 15- 71 14-3 22.2 I7.I 6-5 2.8 I6.61 15-9 15-6 15-3 I.O .6 41-4 6.4 19.6 3.9 23-9 ii. 8 in per cent., is +.496 ±.093. Although the numbers are scanty there is a significant relation between the two. This means that the more unlike the plants are the greater the distinction that is made between the two kinds of pollen. In proportion as the cross-fertilization benefits the immediate progeny in its development the less effective is that pollen in accomplishing the union. The same method of experimentation was applied to another plant, the garden tomato, Lycopersicum esculentum Mill. Ad- vantage was taken of plant characters such that the seedlings could be distinguished in both reciprocal applications. Pollen from a variety with entire leaves with a tall habit of growth was mixed with pollen from a dwarf variety with normal, serrate leaves. Tall stature and normal leaves are dominant so that the cross-fertilized and self-fertilized seedlings from one variety were 1 Number of seeds too few to make averages reliable. 272 D. F. JONES. visible because of differences in leaf formation and in the other variety by habit of growth. Dwarf plants are characteristically shorter and more compact in stems and leaves which gives them a distinct appearance. The plants used were grown from un- pedigreed seed but the tomato is usually self-fertilized and the varieties employed were tested and found to come true to type. Two experiments were made and the results from these are given as pollen mixtures number 21 and 22 in the appendix. The plants from individual fruits are grown separately. The total numbers in the two mixtures are 340 and 272. Differentiation was sharp in the seedling stage in the A lots with either serrate or entire leaves. In the B lots the presence of tall and dwarf seedlings could be easily seen but not all of them could be separated as positively as could be desired. The plants were therefore set in the field and classified after they had grown about two months. They were set out too late to make a satisfactory growth and even at the end of the season classifica- tion was not made with certainty in every case. TABLE V. CORRELATION BETWEEN THE AMOUNT OF HETEROSIS, SHOWN BY THE PER CENT. INCREASE IN WEIGHT OF CROSSED SEEDS, AND THE SELECTIVE ACTION IN FAVOR OF THE PLANTS' OWN POLLEN, r = + .496 ± .093. Per Cent. Increase in Weight. 5 7 9 ii 13 15 17 19 21 23 25 27 29 31 33 35 37 39 41 43 c 13-5 i i 2 io.5 i I 7-5 O 4-5 0 — 1.5 0 + 1-5 ii i i ii 6 4-5 i i 2 7-5 ill 3 io-5 o 13-5 i i 2 i6.5 0 19-5 i I 22.5 0 25-5 0 28.5 0 31-5 0 34-5 o 37-5 o 40-5 o 43-5 0 46-5 ii i 3 O 0 I 20 SELECTIVE FERTILIZATION IN POLLEN MIXTURES. 273 POLLEN MIXTURE No. i. AX A. AXB. BXA. BXB. CX A. CXB. 317 i 319 5 361 10 189 2 224 9 429 IS 270 7 369 10 421 13 389 8 348 26 445 i 332 8 330 6 260 i 1,757 27 1,590 56 1,656 39 1. 21 7.69 o 22.22 •39 6.66 21 2 o 12 6 3 1,738 46 1, 602 44 1.653 42 97.420 2.580 97-330 2.670 97.520 2.480 97-375 2.625 97-375 2.625 97-375 2.625 +•045 -.045 -•045 +.045 +•145 -.145 POLLEN MIXTURE No. 2. AX A. AXB. BXA. BXB. 236 ii 359 42 206 9 353 20 188 IS 380 42 141 3 230 15 194 8 372 39 237 ii 214* 12 232 ii 286 36 167 7 265 19 I5i 3 273 31 188 8 341 29 245 7 2,185 93 3,073 285 2.69 8.00 o 2-34 59 7 0 7 2,133 US 3,080 278 93-630 6.370 91.720 8.280 92.675 7.325 92.675 7.325 + •955 --955 --955 +-955 The results, taken as they stand, are the same as obtained from maize. There is a deviation favoring the plants' own pollen of 2.06 and 6.84 per cent., in the two cases with the prob- ability values .907 and .082 respectively. In the first instance the difference can easily be due to random sampling, in the other the odds are strongly against such an explanation. In view of the fact that differences in germination of the seeds and viability of plants most certainly tend to decrease the proportion of self- fertilized individuals, the data have some value. They should be corroborated by larger numbers using other characters which can be more surely identified before they are as convincing as the results with maize. 274 D. F. JONES. POLLEN MIXTURE No. 3. AX A. A XB. BX A. BXB. 23 3 90 41 54 7 114 28 27 0 27 26 34 i 15 20 18 0 97 56 43 2 73 19 19 O 76 52 4 O 27 8 7 I 50 3 4i 14 66 55 33 5 22 ii 33 17 229 14 764 355 0 0 O 1-75 0 0 0 6 229 14 770 349 94.240 5.760 68.810 31.190 81-525 18.475 81.525 18.475 + 12.715 -12.715 -12.715 + 12.715 POLLEN MIXTURE No. 4. AX A. AXB. BX A. BXB. 189 38 301 67 173 42 208 IO2 165 ii 248 70 64 14 164 I67 41 7 194 38 77 15 254 II/ 213 38 278 49 709 127 1, 860 648 0 -99 -83 1.72 0 i 15 ii 710 126 1,856 652 84.930 15.070 74.000 26.000 79.465 20.535 79-465 20.535 + 5-465 -5-465 -5-465 + 5.465 POLLEN MIXTURE No. 5. Ax A. AXB. BXA. BXB. 23 123 56 143 244 2 3 i 0 0 219 246 313 352 160 8 14 24 19 6 589 98.990 96.885 +2.105 6 I.OIO 3.115 -2.105 1,290 94.780 96.885 -2.105 7i 5-220 3-II5 + 2.105 SELECTIVE FERTILIZATION IN POLLEN MIXTURES. 275 POLLEN MIXTURE No, 6. AX A. AXB. BXA. E/B. 40 71 184 129 — — 0 2.63 — — o 3 40 71 187 126 36.04 . 63.96 59-74 40.26 47.89 52.11 47.89 52.11 -11.85 + 11.85 + 11.85 -11.85 POLLEN MIXTURE No. 7. AX A. A X B. B X A. BXB. 23 89 253 188 104 189 185 212 84 117 1 66 174 H5 139 118 156 123 190 116 169 109 158 134 148 23 89 1.507 1,840 20.540 79.460 45.030 54.970 32.785 67.215 32.785 67.215 -12.245 + 12.245 + 12.245 -12.245 POLLEN MIXTURE No. 8. AX A. AXB. BXA. BXB. 96 53 173 108 158 112 98 69 154 60 74 83 136 47 102 102 1 06 38 175 131 103 29 199 147 135 65 109 79 89 53 196 89 130 47 142 153 60 ii 203 131 128 49 117 93 63 38 52 52 69 29 1 66 idf) 87 34 109 7') 186 67 83 72 59 3i 138 86 103 54 133 138 77 34 76 75 124 5i 45 4i 58 18 119 116 64 36 75 108 2,185 956 2,584 2,058 0 o o 1.69 o o 0 35 2,185 956 2,619 2,023 69.56 30.44 56.42 43.58 62.99 37-01 62.99 37-01 +6-57 -6-57 -6-57 +6.57 276 D. F. JONES. POLLEN MIXTURE No. 9. A X A. AX B. BXA. B XB. 277 149 209 US 116 69 173 99 168 69 264 156 135 78 188 114 151 52 160 IOI 78 30 2IO 142 128 71 172 134 254 IO2 191 160 286 147 166 112 208 97 195 134 185 88 196 107 H3 55 175 108 208 117 207 105 277 130 128 97 92 73 123 76 123 86 2,584 1-254 2,972 1,919 2-54 2-54 1.67 0 66 32 50 o 2,550 1,288 2,922 1,969 66.44 33-56 59-74 40.26 63.09 36.91 63.09 36.91 +3-35 -3-35 -3-35 +3-35 POLLEN MIXTURE No. 10. AX A. AX B. BXA. BXB. 344 331 189 397 200 272 195 447 284 250 189 522 275 282 233 399 169 430 1,103 I.I35 975 2,195 1.71 o 1.69 1.71 19 0 16 38 1,084 1,154 997 2,173 48.440 51-560 31.450 68.550 39-945 60.055 39-945 60.055 +8.495 -8.495 -8-495 +8.495 POLLEN MIXTURE No. n. AXA. AXB. BXA. BXB. 67 57 209 130 385 203 290 199 279 265 386 191 341 312 452 260 1,505 1,097 .88 0 0 o 4 0 0 o 448 264 1.505 1,097 62.92 37.08 57.84 42.16 60.38 39.62 60.38 39.62 +2-54 -2.54 -2-54 +2.54 SELECTIVE FERTILIZATION IN POLLEN MIXTURES. 277 POLLEN MIXTURE No. 12. AX A. AXB. BXA. BXB. 552 196 218 1 06 86 67 197 92 265 in 163 69 253 114 105 46 182 61 66 56 401 155 L739 704 749 369 .86 0 0 0 15 0 o o 1,724 719 749 369 70.57 29-43 66.99 33-01 68.78 31.22 68.78 31.22 + 1-79 -1.79 -1.79 + 1.79 POLLEN MIXTURE No. 13. A x A. AxB. BXA. BxB. 37 52 14 II 17 87 13 18 II 16 17 II 28 86 6 4 50 9i 3i 8 42 59 14 13 185 391 95 65 32.120 67.880 59-370 40.630 45-745 54-255 45-745 54-255 -13-625 + 13-625 + 13-625 -13-625 POLLEN MIXTURE No. 14. AXA. A x B. Bx A. BxB. 41 91 83 70 139 13 35 39 27 36 58 35 46 17 31 15 IO 6 424 73-S70 72.715 + I-I55 ISO 26.130 27.285 -I-I55 156 71.560 72.715 -I- 155 62 28.440 27-285 + I-I55 POLLEN MIXTURE No. 15. Ax A. AxB. BxA. BxB. 309 II 3 Ill 415 II O 53 393 9 I 7i 326 7 I H3 276 2 O 67 135 I 2 90 400 4 355 2 2,609 47 (7) (505) 14 498 98.23 1.77 2-73 97.27 50.48 49-52 50.48 49-52 +47-75 -47-75 -47-75 +47-75 278 D. F. JONES. POLLEN MIXTURE No. 16. Ax A. AxB. BxA. BxB. 285 5 12 355 187 i 8 350 251 2 8 408 4 226 5 117 723 8 (37) (1.456) 74 1,419 98.910 1.090 4.960 95.040 51-935 48.065 Si-9.15 48.065 +46.975 -46.975 -46.975 +46.975 POLLEN MIXTURE No. 17. A x A. A x B. Bx A. BxB. 255 i 4 49 1 66 o 0 5i 185 i 222 0 22O I 255 o 1.303 3 4 IOO 99-77 -23 3-85 96.15 51.81 48.19 51.81 48.19 +47-96 -47.96 -47-96 +47.96 POLLEN MIXTURE No. 18. Ax A. A x B. BxA. BxB. 3 5 I 368 i 16 O 432 0 320 0 336 O 39 o 78 4 21 I 1-573 16.00 84.00 0.06 99-94 8.03 91.97 8.03 91.97 + 7-97 -7-97 -7-97 + 7-97 POLLEN MIXTURE No. 19. A XA. AxB. BxA. BxB. C x A C x B. 2O 16 41 206 75 213 89 53 36 147 7i 124 68 86 42 200 61 315 55 47 45 178 69 39 29 150 45 20 63 156 52 37 30 88 65 8 57 218 65 86 528 392 343 1.343 207 652 57-390 42.610 20.340 79.660 24.100 75.900 38.865 6I.I35 38.865 6i.i35 38.865 6LI35 + 18.525 -18.525 -18.525 + 18.525 -14-765 + 14-765 SELECTIVE FERTILIZATION IN POLLEN MIXTURES. 279 POLLEN MIXTURE No. 20. A x A. Ax B. Bx A. Bx B. C x A. C x B. 172 II 193 86 40 52 43 6 104 58 46 56 165 IS II? 86 Si 55 38 2 80 45 2 5 89 4 47 17 103 8 128 52 82 12 81 47 19 O 94 60 53 4 92 30 33 o 99 43 69 13 75 46 3i 2 64 35 89? 77 I-I74 605 139 168 92.09 7.91 65.99 34.01 45.28 54-72 79.04 20.96 79.04 20.96 79-04 20.96 + I3-OS -13-05 -13-05 + I3-05 -33-76 +33.76 POLLEN MIXTURE No. 21. A x A. Ax B. Bx A. B x B. 39 16 17 25 12 14 I 5 32 25 36 18 14 18 97 73 54 48 57.06 42.94 52.94 47.06 55-00 45.00 55-00 45.00 +2.06 — 2.06 — 2.06 +2.06 POLLEN MIXTURE No. 22. A x A. A x B. Bx A. Bx B. 7 IO 107 22 27 103 13 7 17 27 124 44-93 38.09 +6.84 152 55-07 6I.9I -6.84 20 31-25 38.09 -6.84 44 68.75 61.91 +6.84 PREVIOUS INVESTIGATIONS ON SELECTIVE FERTILIZATION. From the work of Kolreuter, Herbert, Gartner, Darwin, M tiller, Knuth and others we are familiar with the phenomenon of self-sterility in plants in which the individual's own pollen is wholly incapable of functioning on the plant by which it is pro- duced although perfectly developed and able to fulfill its duties when brought to other plants of the same species. Numerous investigators have been giving attention to this problem in recent times. East and Park (1917) have made a noteworthy contribu- 28O D. F. JONES. tion to its solution and give a complete resume of the work which has been done on this subject. They have been able to demonstrate that groups exist within which the individuals are all both self-sterile and cross-sterile, but any member of one group is perfectly fertile with any member of any other group. These investigators find that there are about 100 well-endorsed instances of self-sterility in plants scattered over some 35 families. Undoubtedly this discrimination is a means to promote cross- fertilization of approximately the same significance as floral contrivances, dichogamy and dioecism. Even though widespread in its occurrence self-sterility is a special adaptive process ful- filling a particular function. It is directly opposite in its effect to the results found in maize which shows no self-sterility of the type found in Nicotiana and other genera. At least no clear cases are known of maize pollen, which is unable to fertilize the plants which produced it, being able to fertilize other plants. Darwin has furnished many instances of self-sterility. In addition he reports some experiments which led him to believe that even when a plant was normally self-fertile that pollen from unrelated plants of the same species was prepotent over the plant's own pollen. In discussing means which insure flowers being fertilized with pollen from distinct plants, he says: "We now come to a far more general and therefore more important means by which the mutual fertilization of distinct plants is effected, namely, the fertilizing power of pollen from another variety or individual being greater than that of a plant's own pollen. The simplest and best knowTn case of prepotent action in pollen, though it does not bear directly on our present subject( is that of a plant's own pollen over that from a distinct species. If pollen from a distinct species be placed on the stigma of a castrated flower, and then after the interval of several hours, pollen from the same species be placed on the stigma, the effects of the former are wholly obliterated, excepting in some rare cases. If twro varieties are treated in the same manner, the result is analogous, though of directly opposite nature; for pollen from any other variety is often or generally prepotent over that from the same flower" ("Cross- and Self-Fertilization, " pp. 391-392). These statements were based on observations and experiments SELECTIVE FERTILIZATION IN POLLEN MIXTURES. 28l with various cultivated plants. Different types of crucifers— kohl-rabi, borccoli, Brussels sprouts, cabbage — were grown near each other and the seed resulting from pollination at will, when grown, showed a large amount of intercrossing. The observation was also made with different varieties of the radish, Raphanus sativus. These plants are all partially self-sterile so that cross- fertilization is expected in somewhat greater degree than would result from random pollination. Mixing was also shown by plants which are generally self-fertile such as tulip, hyacinth, anemone, ranunculus, strawberry, orange, rhododendron, rub- barb. The fact that vicinism occurs when varieties of these plants are grown together is established by such observations but this does not prove that one type of pollen is prepotent over the other. Somewhat more significant results were obtained from two other species. Mimulus luteus was found to be highly fertile when insects were excluded. Uncastrated flowers of a constant whitish variety were artificially pollinated by a yellowish variety and of the 28 resulting plants all had yellowish flowers so that the "pollen of the yellow variety completely over- whelmed that from the mother plant." A crimson variety of Iberis umbellate, which was self-fertile, was crossed with a pink variety, the pollen being applied to uncastrated flowers as before upon the stigmas of which he saw abundant pollen presumably from the same flowers. Out of 30 plants raised 24 showed themselves to be crossed by the altered color of their flowers. Obviously experiments such as these are not sufficient to establish the prepotency of foreign pollen in self-fertile plants. A number of conclusions might be drawn from such results. The cross-fertilized seeds may have germinated better and the plants grown from them survived in greater numbers. The types may not have been as constant for their flower color as Darwin supposed or the ovules may not have been receptive at the time the plant's own pollen was available but were when the foreign pollen was applied. Taken as they stand the results do indicate a prepotency of pollen from dissimilar plants and it would be desirable to investigate this effect with these species using mixed pollen in reciprocal applications as employed with maize and the tomato. 282 D. F. JONES. Darwin knew of many cases of total self-sterility and was so convinced of the necessity for cross-fertilization that he was easily persuaded from these observations that a prepotency of pollen from unrelated plants did exist since he supposed this enabled a plant to choose between its own and unrelated pollen when both were brought at the same time to the stigmas by insects or other agencies. So plausible have been the arguments in favor of such an assumption that the prepotency of germ cells from individuals of somewhat different constitution, even where com- plete self-fertility exists, has been accepted as an established fact and incorporated in textbooks on biology. Similarly inconclusive experiments have been performed with animals. Marshall (1910) artificially impregnated a pure bred dog with a mixture of equal quantities of seminal fluids from the same breed and from a mongrel of unknown ancestry. Of the four young which resulted one died early, and three resembled somewhat the mongrel sire. Marshall cites another instance in which a dog copulated with a member of the same breed and two days later with a sire of different type. Out of three puppies one was pure bred and two half-breeds. These cases, according to this writer, indicated a selective action favoring dissimilar rather than related spermatozoa. King (1918) mentions some preliminary experiments with albino and wild gray rats in which advantage was given to the former, yet the results tended to show a prepotency of the latter, so that there was apparently a selective action favoring the out-cross. The details of these experiments are not given. In attempting to determine whether or not a selective action exists small numbers can never be more than suggestive and unless the mixture is applied at the same time to both types furnishing the sperm cells there is no way of estimating the rela- tive proportions of the two kinds of fertilizing elements present in the mixture which are capable of functioning. Furthermore a constant excess of cross-fertilized individuals over the others may be due to the greater viability of the hybrids and hence there will be a selective elimination of zygotes but not necessarily slective fertilization. Hyde (1914) compared the matings of different types of Drosophila within the strains and in reciprocal SELECTIVE FERTILIZATION IN POLLEN MIXTURES. 283 crosses. The dissimilar unions gave greatly increased numbers in both reciprocal combinations. However, the type of mating did not influence the number of eggs laid and there is no proof that cross-fertilization occurred more readily than self-fertiliza- tion. The results show that the cross-fertilized eggs hatched better and the offspring survived in greater numbers, a result which is easily understood since there were lethal factors involved in the material worked with. The only evidence from the animal side of a definite selective action comparable to the many instances of self-sterility in plants is the well-known case of self-impotency in dona intes- tinalis (Castle, 1896). Morgan (1905, '07, '10) has experimented with this organism and has found that the self-sterility is not always complete. Material gathered on the Pacific coast showed somewhat greater receptiveness to the individual's own sperm than eggs of the same species at Woods Hole which were almost entirely unresponsive to sperm from the same individual which produced the eggs. In another ascidian, Cynthia partida, he found that self-fertilization takes place frequently but the sperm of unrelated individuals is more effective. A third species, Molgula manhattensis is self-fertilized as readily as cross-fertilized. From this it seems that incompatibility of uniting gametes as a means of insuring cross-fertilization exists in various grades of effectiveness. Even in extreme cases the degree of self-sterility may be modified by internal and external conditions. In Nico- tiana East and Park find that self-fertilization sometimes takes place towards the end of the growing period when the vigor of the plants is reduced. DISCUSSION. As far as the writer knows the results obtained from maize stand alone among plants in showing a selective action unfavor- able to fertilization by sperm from individuals of different hereditary constitution. The handicap placed upon the foreign pollen is proportional to the germinal unlikeness. If the unequal effect is due to a slower growth of the pollen tube through the tissues of style the selective action may be restricted to plants,1 1 E. C. Miller (Jour. Agric. Research, Vol. 18. pp. 255-266, Dec. 1919), has recently made a detailed study of fertilization in maize and finds that from many 284 D. F. JONES. and would also not be surprising to find that the phenomenon is greater in maize than in any other species for the reason that in this plant the pollen tubes have a larger distance to traverse to accomplish fertilization than in any other form known to the writer. The stigmatic hairs of maize are scattered along a filamentous style which continues to grow until fertilization takes place. The structure withers and dries shortly after poll'nation takes place. Pollen may adhere at any point along the filament. The total length of style through which the pollen tube grows is normally from 10 to 20 centimeters but in extreme cases may be as great as 50 or more. It will be worth while to see whether or not the selective effect is more pronounced when the styles are long than when short. Such an experiment can be easily carried out and would give some indications as to whether the handicap is placed during pollen tube growth or after the sperm nuclei are brought to the egg. The lessened ability of moderately different types to fertilize is in line with the impossibility of effecting unions between widely separated forms. In such cases the prevention of fertilization is sometimes due to mechanical difficulties in the way of bringing sperm cells to the eggs but even when this is accomplished there still exists a firm barrier which prevents the passage through the egg membrane. The differential effect demonstrated in maize may be simply a reduced manifestation of this phenomenon. It is possible that the experiments on anaphyllaxis may throw some light on this problem. It is known that foreign proteins when injected in animal tissues may have a toxic effect and excite an extreme irritability so that in repeated doses they may cause markedly injurious results. By this means it is possible to distinguish between proteins of very slight differences in composition. Since the differences in protoplasmic substances between the types in which a selective action is shown seem to be small there may be some relation between the two phenomena. However, the male gametophyte growing upon the stigmas is, in pollen tubes which start to grow down the style only one tube in every case in nearly 100 observations was seen to reach the ovary cavity. This indicates that the differential fertilizing power is determined by the rate of pollen tube growth and not after the sperm nuclei have been brought to the egg. SELECTIVE FERTILIZATION IN POLLEN MIXTURES. 285 a way, merely a parasitic organism. It would be difficult to find evidence from true parasites that they are restrained by their hosts in proportion as they are genetically dissimilar although in most cases the differences are so great that probably there is no basis for comparison. The only evidence which has any direct bearing on the problem comes from grafting experiments. It is well known with plants that the affinity of stock and cion is directly proportional to their phylogenetic similarity. In animals the same rule holds, and very fine distinctions are manifested. Morgan (1910) cites the results of Schoene in which the skin of the mouse is readily grafted back upon the same individual or member of the same litter but not upon unrelated mice. Such results as these are quite similar to the greater receptiveness of plants to their own pollen. For some time there has been current in biological literature the hypothesis that heterogeneity in the structure of organisms favors growth and reproduction and conversely that homogeneity , is unfavorable. This is a heritage from Darwinism and has appeared again and again in theories of rejuvenation, vigor derived from crossing, and selective fertilization. It has been stated in many different ways but in general terms it amounts to the supposition that similarity in protoplasmic structure brought about by a line of similar ancestors is not conducive to physio- logical efficiency and that the differences brought about by the union of unlike elements and the consequent lack of balance stimulates growth. The only basis for this hypothesis is found in the necessity, in most cases, for the union of two differentiated sex cells to start the development in the egg. The attempt to argue by analogy than the union of dissimilar protoplasms is, in itself, an immediate physiological benefit is not supported by the facts and is founded upon fallacious reasoning. Rejuvenation in vegetatively reproduced organisms by sexual union is no longer looked upon as beneficial in destroying simi- larity in structure. That the process of forming gametes and their reunion may bring about a reorganization of the proto- plasmic substances and an elimination of waste products so as to result in greater growth seems quite plausible. But the 286 D. F. JONES. significance of such a process is not to be looked for in the bringing together of differentiated germplasms. The advantage derived from cross-fertilization is now under- stood as a phenomenon of inheritance and the older hypothesis of the stimulation of heterozygosis is no longer needed. Accord- ing to present theory homozygous factor combinations are more efficient than heterozygous combinations of the same factors. In the lowest organisms which are illustrative of a primitive sexuality there is direct proof that the union of different indi- viduals does not result in an increased developmental efficiency. Jennings (1913) finds that in Paramecium, in the generations immediately following conjugation, there is a slowing down in the rate of division. The advantage derived from the pairing of individuals is the greater elasticity in adaptiveness resulting from the mixing of different germ plasms giving to some of the descendants a greater chance for survival. Pearl (1907) has found that in this same organism there is a tendency for like forms to conjugate due to mechanical hindrances to the pairing of individuals dissimilar in form. This has been substantiated by Jennings (1911). Also in gastropods Crozier (1918) has demonstrated that assortative mating takes place between indi- viduals of the same size and this, he considers, results in a greater number of offspring than there would be if random pairing was the rule. It has been proven by Pearson and Lee (1903) that assortative mating occurs in man. This conclusion is reached after extensive investigation in the inheritance of physical char- acters in which they have found that there are positive correla- tions between husband and wife with respect to stature, span of arms, and length of forearm. Moreover they have shown that homogamy is a factor favorable to fertility. Parents with like characters are more productive of offspring. This is an impor- tant observation and supports the main thesis of this paper. The occurrence of homogamy in such widely diverse forms of life as the higher plants, protozoa, mollusca and man cannot fail to have significance. The importance of discriminate isolation has long been recognized since it supplies a necessary factor in divergent evolution. The existence of such an assortative action can now be looked for in all forms of life since evidence has been SELECTIVE FERTILIZATION IN POLLEN MIXTURES. 287 produced from the corners of the phylogenetical triangle. The evolutionary significance of this phenomenon has been ably reviewed by Peal in the above citation so that it is not useful to go into that phase of the problem in detail here. It is interesting to note that the results from maize fulfill his expectation as in conclusion he says, "the fact that we find such a high degree of homogamy in a protozoan form like Paramecium strongly sug- gests the possibility that in higher organisms there may be assortative mating of the gametes in the process of fertilization. Should such a homogamy occur it would probably be of far greater importance than any assorted mating of the somas." While the selective agency in plants does not differentiate be- tween gametes produced by one type in the end the result is the same. Individuals with like characters tend to be brought to- gether and virtually to be set apart from the general population. Biological investigations unite to show that the importance of sex is to make organisms more plastic in adaptability. The advantages have been so great that sexual reproduction is now established as the dominant method for the renewal of organism in both kingdoms at the expense of economy and speed of multi- plication. It is not strange then that accessory devices have been developed to insure the fulfillment of the function for which so much has been expended. Self-sterility or self-impotency is one of the means developed to -serve this purpose. The reverse phenomenon, that of self-prepotency, so far is shown among plants only by maize. It would be surprising to find it limited to this one species. Is it not more likely to be a general manifestation? As a fundamental principle it may apply even to those organisms which show self-sterility, this latter being a special adaptation entirely overcoming the handicap placed upon unfamiliar gametes in order to make certain the advantages which exogamy holds out. One cannot insist upon such an assumption with evidence from only one or possibly two species. But the evidence, limited though it may be for the present, is one more indication that homogeneity, similarity, likeness, familiarity, or however it may be described, in proto- plasmic structure is consistent with and favorable to the highest physiological efficiency. 288 D. F. JONES. In maintaining two opposing tendencies Nature is not neces- sarily working at cross-purposes. Biparental inheritance with the inclination towards exogamy serves to bring about plasticity. The preferential mating of similar kind operates to make this mixing discriminative. Probably it is not yet time to reconcile completely these two contradictory forces. It may at least be held that assortative mating which favors the pairing of like with like has some importance in evolution since it is an agency in orthogenetic changes. Perceptive reproduction of this kind tends to hold organisms in certain paths once a break from the common type has been made. Having been demonstrated in three of the farthest separated branches of the organic world- angiosperms, protozoa, and man — homogamy may take a some- what more authoritative part in evolutionary theory. LITERATURE CITED. Belling, J. '14 The Mode of Inheritance of Semi-sterility in the Offspring of Certain Hybrid Plants. Zeitsch. f. ind. Abst. u. Vererb., Vol. 12, pp. 303-342. Castle, W. E. '96 The Early Embryology of dona inteslinalis Flemming (L.). Bull. Mus. Comp. Zool., Harvard University, Vol. 27, pp. 201-280. '03 The Heredity of Sex. Bull. Mus. Comp. Zool., Harvard University, Vol. 40, pp. 189-218. Crozier, W. J. '18 Assertive mating in a Nudibranch, Chromodoris Zebra Heilprin. Journ. Exper. Zool., Vol. 27, pp. 247-292. Cuenot, L. '08 Sur quelques anomalies apparentes des proportions Mendeliennes (6e note). Arch. Zool. Exp. et Gen., Vol. 9, pp. 7-15- Davis, B. M. '15 The Test of a Pure Species of CEnothera. Proc. Am. Phil. Soc., Vol. 54, pp. 226-245. '17 A Criticism of the Evidence for the Mutation Theory of DeVries from the Behavior of Species of CEnothera in Crosses and in Selfed Lines. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sciences, Vol. 3, pp. 704-710. East, E. M. '19 Studies on Self-Sterility. IV. Selective Fertilization. Genetics, Vol. 4, pp. 346-355- East, E. M., and Hayes, H. K. 'u Inheritance in Maize. Connecticut Agr. Exp. Sta. Bull. 167, pp. 1-142. East, E. M., and Park, J. B. '17 Studies on Self-sterility. I. The Behavior of Self-sterile Plants. Genetics, Vol. 2, pp. 505-609. SELECTIVE FERTILIZATION IN POLLEN MIXTURES. 289 Elderton, W. P. '01 Tables for Testing the Goodness of Fit of Theory to Observation. Bio- metrika, Vol. i, pp. 155-163. Hyde, R. H. '14 Fertility and Sterility in Drosophila ampelophila. Journ. Exper. Zool., Vol. 17, Nos. 1-4. Jennings, H. S. 'n Assertive Mating, Variability and Inheritance of Size, in the Conjugation of Paramecium. Journ. Exper. Zool., Vol. n, pp. 1-134. '13 The Effect of Conjugation in Paramecium. Journ. Exper. Zool., Vol. I4> PP- 279-391. Jones, D. F. '18 The Effects of Inbreeding and Crossbreeding upon Development. Con- necticut Agr. Exp. Sta. Bull. 207, pp. i-ioo, pis. 1-12. Kempton, J. H. '19 Inheritance of Waxy Endosperm in Maize. U. S. Dept. of Agr., Bur. P. I. Bull. 754, PP- i-99- King, H. D. '18 Studies in Inbreeding. III. The Effects of Inbreeding, with Selection, on the Sex Ratio of the Albino Rat. Journ. Exper. Zool., Vol. 27, pp. 1-35. Marshall, F. H. A. '10 The Physiology of Reproduction. London. Morgan, T. H. '05 Some Further Experiments in Self-fertilization in Ciona. BIOL. BULL., Vol. 8, pp. 313-330. '07 Experimental Zoology. New York. '10 Cross and Self-fertilization in Ciona intestinalis. Arch. f. Entwicklungs- mech. d. Org., Vol. 30, pp. 206-234. Muller, H. J. '18 Genetic Variability, Twin Hybrids, and Constant Hybrids, in a Case of Balanced Lethal Factors. Genetics, Vol. 3, pp. 422-499. Pearl, R. '07 A Biometrical Study of Conjugation in Paramecium. Biometrika, Vol. 5, pp. 213-297. Pearson, K., and Lee, A. '03 On the Laws of Inheritance in Man. I. The Inheritance of Physical Characters. Biometrika, Vol. 2, pp. 357-462. '03 Assortative Mating in Man. Biometrika, Vol. 2, pp. 481-498. STUDIES ON THE CELLS OF CATTLE WITH SPECIAL REFERENCE TO SPERMATOGENESIS, OOGONIA, AND SEX-DETERMINATION. J. E. WODSEDALEK, ZOOLOGY DEPARTMENT, UNIVERSITY OF IDAHO. l CONTENTS. I. Introduction 290 II. Material and Methods 293 III. General Arrangement of the Male Germinal Cells 294 IV. Spermatogenesis 294 1. Spermatogonia 295 2. Primary Spermatocytes 296 3. Secondary Spermatocytes 297 4. Spermatids 297 V. Dimorphism in the Spermatozoa 297 VI. Oogonia 298 VII. Chromosomes in Somatic Cells 300 VIII. Sex- Chromosomes in Relation to Sex-Determination 302 IX. Sex Ratio in Cattle 303 X. Sex-Limited Inheritance in Cattle 304 XI. The Free-Martin 305 XII. Summary 305 I. INTRODUCTION. Sex-determination with its many attendant problems has always been a subject of great interest to practical animal breeders; and the art of breeding has always been replete with rules by which the sex ratio might be shifted in various ways to the advantage of the breeder. These rules, however, have been founded upon inadequate evidence and unsound reasoning. The most common beliefs in regard to sex control have from time to time been founded on heat relations, some maintaining that the products of conception in early heat were more often males, others that they were more often females. Pearl and Parshley ('13) have published data on sex-determination in cattle with the following conclusions: (i) That as the time of coitus approaches the end of the cestrous period there is a pro- 1 This research was supported in part by the Adams Fund of the Idaho Experi- ment Station. 290 STUDIES ON THE CELLS OF CATTLE. 2QI gressive increase in the proportion of male young born. (2) That in the extreme case this increase in the proportion of male births is probably statistically significant and not to be attributed to errors of random sampling. (3) That these modifications of the sex ratio cannot be attributed to age differences or to any other factor yet suggested. More recent and extensive data, however, according to Pearl ('17) make the relation of time of service to sex extremely doubtful. He says: "The apparent relation between these two factors, which is believed by many breeders to exist and which our earlier statistics appeared to indicate, seems now to be purely accidental, and to have arisen only because of the com- parative meagerness of the statistics on which the matter was discussed." Sex in cattle is a matter of heredity, as is shown by the results of this investigation, and remains a matter beyond the control of the breeder; its ultimate control is problematical. However, the suggestion of experimentally separating the two types of spermatozoa, or destroying the one type without impair- ing the nature of the other, does not appear to be entirely hope- less. If this can be done, the problem of sex control will be comparatively simple, since artificial insemination can be resorted to. It appears to be a well-established fact that sex is determined at the time of fertilization. Sex, like other characters of the individual, has a definite factorial basis; and the factorial con- stitution of the individual with respect to sex as well as to other characters is fixed by the constitution of the two gametes which unite to form the zygote. The dimorphic condition among the spermatozoa in many of the lower animal forms is well known. Guyer pointed it out in the rat ('10), and man ('10) ; other investigators have reported it in several other mammals. Among the domestic mammals the same condition has been clearly demonstrated in the pig (Wod- sedalek, '13) and in the horse (Wodsedalek, '14). This study on the sex cells of cattle shows that two types of spermatozoa are also produced in the bull ; the one type at the time of fertilization determining maleness and the other type determining femaleness. The spermatozoan which determines femaleness is somewhat 2Q2 J. E. WODSEDALEK. larger owing to the presence of a large sex-chromosome or acces- sory element which is lacking in the type producing maleness. Since it has been found in this study that the oogonia of cattle possess two sex-chromosomes it is safe to conclude, in view of our knowledge of oogenesis in general, that all of the mature ova carry the reduced number or one sex-chromosome. If the ovum is fertilized by a sperm which lacks the sex-chromosome the resulting zygote naturally possess only one sex-chromosome and, therefore,. develops into a male. This is evidenced by the fact that the somatic as well as the germ cells of the male possess a single accessory element. On the other hand, if the ovum is fertilized by a sperm possessing the sex-chromosome the resulting zygote possesses two sex-chromosomes and, therefore, develops into a female. The somatic as well as the germ cells of the female possess the two accessory elements which is in exact accord with expectations. The results presented in this paper were obtained through a most careful and critical study extending over a period of more than four years. The problem was started in the spring of 1915 and every summer since and a great deal of the spare time in the intervening school years, with the exception of the year 1918, has been devoted to it. In the aggregate this means a total of about fifteen months of continuous work. Practically all of the results presented in this paper were at hand at the end of the first two and a half years of study. Over four hundred pencil sketches were made of the various cells, especially those in mitosis, with the aid of the camera lucida and the location of each, as indicated by the mechanical stage, was carefully recorded in a separate booklet. A brief summary of the results of the study was written up and all of the material was put away for a whole year (1918) during which the summer months and other spare time was devoted to the study of the sex cells in another mammal. No effort was made to remember any of the results of the study on the sex cells in cattle. In the meantime, considerable new cattle material was obtained and hundreds of new slides were made by the departmental technician. At the end of the year the new cattle material was studied and the results were carefully recorded. The original STUDIES ON THE CELLS OF CATTLE. 2Q3 material was then reinvestigated and new sketches were made of many of the cells, the location of which was recorded before. The results obtained through the study of new slides prepared by the technician, as well as those obtained through the second study of my original material were then compared with the original results. Several hundred different germinal and somatic cells from both sexes were carefully checked up. Especial atten- tion was paid to the number of the ordinary chromosomes and to the sex-chromosomes. Only in a comparatively few cases was there any discrepancy in the interpretation of the nature and number of the ordinary chromosomes. Conditions pertaining to the sex-chromosomes were corroborated in every instance. I was further checked up on my chromosome counts by several of my assistants and senior pre-medical students who had con- siderable training in microscopic anatomy. In general, these men who knew nothing of my own interpretations, corroborated my counts in a surprisingly large number of instances. II. MATERIAL AND METHODS. All of the material used in this investigation with the excep- tion of some ovaries was obtained through the courtesy of the management of the Hagan and Gushing Packing Plant which adjoins the University farm. Some excellent ovarian tissue was given to me by my colleague Dr. A. R. Hahner, formerly professor of veterinary science of the University of Idaho. The ovaries were removed from two five-months-old heifers of the university herd. Testes were obtained from seven mature bulls and one male foetus of five months and from six smaller foetuses varying from two to eight and one half inches in length. The ovaries were obtained from four heifers and as many cows, and from five small foetuses, varying from two and one half to seven inches in length. In addition to this six small embryos were sectioned. Many slides were also made of various somatic structures from the small foetuses of both sexes. Several fixing fluids were tried on the testicular material, in- cluding Hermann's, Gilson's, Flemming's, and Bouin's. Bouin's fluid used straight or with slight modifications, at 38° C. was the most universally successful fixing agent. When used at some- 294 J- E. WODSEDALEK. what lower temperatures, the material showed little or no modi- fications. When Bouin's fluid was modified, the alteration took the form of the addition of a small amount of chromic acid, or urea, or the reduction of the percentage of acetic acid. All of these slight modifications gave very good results in the testicular material, and the ovarian, embryological and foetal tissues appeared to be best when fixed in the fluid modified with chromic acid. The cold method (Hance '17) was also tried in two instances with the testicular tissue but with less success. And while I have not tried this out on the cattle tissue myself, I have every assurance that the laboratory technician carried out the process with great care. In the study of the male germ cells, smears as well as sections were used. Many stains and counter stains were tried. Iron- haematoxylin when used alone was found to be the most satis- factory. All of the figures represented in this paper were made from material stained in this manner. III. GENERAL ARRANGEMENT OF THE MALE GERMINAL CELLS. The structure of the testes of the bull is similar to that of the other well-known mammals and bears a great resemblance to the conditions found in the testes of the horse (Wodsedalek '14). The interstitial cells as in the horse are small and fewer in number in comparison with their large size and great abundance in the testes of the pig. The size of the seminiferous tubules, as well as the general size of the various germinal cells, however, corre- sponds to the condition found in the pig. The usual types of cells, (i) spermatogonia, (2) primary spermatocytes, (3) second- ary spermatocytes, (4) spermatids, and (5) spermatozoa in various stages of development, are present in great abundance. IV. SPERMATOGENESIS. In general the spermatogenesis of the bull corresponds to that of the pig and the horse. Since many of the finer cytological points are given in detail in the papers on the pig and the horse (Wodsedalek '13 and '14) they are omitted here to avoid un- necessary duplication. And while all of the finer details involved in a thorough piece of work in spermatogenesis were carefully STUDIES ON THE CELLS OF CATTLE. 2Q5 studied in this animal, only the phases pretaining to the chromo- some numbers and their behavior are emphasized in this paper. i. Spermatogonia. The spermatogonia usually lie in a single layer next to the wall of the tubule, though occasionally some of the cells are crowded out, thus forming a second layer which is always very irregular. The cells which undergo the last spermatogonial division (Figs. 5-12) are usually beyond the first layer, though occasionally they may be found next to the tubule wall along the entire section of the tubule. At times the cells are far apart, in which case they are flattened out on the tubule wall. The cells also differ considerably in size and appearance, depending on the stage of development they are in. During the resting stage a large nucleolus is invariably present. As a rule it assumes a somewhat heart-shaped appearance; especially is this true in the larger cells and in those in which the chromosomes begin to form. At the conclusion of the resting stage numerous large chromatin granules appear and arrange themselves along fine threads in an entangled mass. The chro- mosomes soon become distinct and mitotic figures are fairly numerous. And while, as a rule, there is considerable over- lapping and massing of the chromosomes in the early spermato- gonial divisions, hundreds of cells were found in which there was little or no overlapping, making accurate counts possible. Thirty-seven chromosomes appear in the late prophase of the spermatogonial division (Fig. i). Thirty-six of these are vari- ously shaped, mainly oblong or slightly curved, and differ somewhat in size. One which is much larger is triangular in form or heart-shaped. This is the accessory or sex-chromosome, and is the same thing as the large nucleolus which appears in the resting stages. This point is certain, as. the body can be easily traced through the various stages of the cells. A similar condi- tion was reported by Guyer ('10) in man, Wodsedalek in the pig ('13) and in the horse ('14). Several other investigators have reported it in other forms since. During division each chromo- some, including the sex-chromosome, divides in two (Figs. 3 and 4). 296 J. E. WODSEDALEK. In the last spermatogonial cells the chromosomes appear in a dense mass. The cells gradually increase in size and great expan- sion takes place in the nuclei (Figs. 5-9). The chromosomes become decidedly distinct and surprisingly well segregated throughout the spherical nucleus (Figs. 10 and n). The cells in these stages are numerous and beautiful. The chromosomes appear to be dense in structure and are thicker than those of the early spermatogonial cells. Several of the chromosomes are almost spherical. They are so evenly distributed that hundreds of accurate counts can be made within a short time. This condition prevailed in all of the mature testes studied and the cells were especially numerous in three two-year-old bulls. Even in somewhat stale tissue (Fig. 12) the chromosomes in these cells appear to remain well segregated. Altogether over one thousand accurate counts were made in these cells alone. 2. Primary Spermatocyles. The sex-chromosome can invariably be seen in the spermato- cytes where it retains its individuality (Fig. 13). Just how pairing takes place in these cells cannot be stated with certainty. When the chromosomes appear for division they are of the reduced number and bivalent in nature. The thirty-six ordinary chromosomes pair while the sex-chromosome remains unpaired and can easily be distinguished from the others (Figs. 14-18). It occasionally shows its double nature in the late prophase (Fig. 17), and more frequently in the later stages of the primary spermatocyte division (Figs. 22, 24, 25, 27, 28 and 29). During the primary spermatocyte division the sex-chromosome usually passes to one pole in advance of the other chromosomes (Figs. 19-25). This unequal division of the chromosomes in these cells (Figs. 19-29) gives rise to two different types of secondary spermatocytes. The one type containing the eighteen ordinary chromosomes plus the sex-chromosome. Just before division is complete the chromosomes become loosely paired (Figs. 28 and 29). This peculiar behavior of the chromosomes is apparently quite common in mammalian tissue. STUDIES ON THE CELLS OF CATTLE. 3. Secondary Spermatocytes. No resting stage occurs in the secondary spermatocyte. This condition also occurs in the horse and is frequently found in the pig, according to my former studies. The secondary spermato- cytes divide soon after they are formed and not infrequently the spindles are formed in the two cells resulting from the first spermatocyte division while they are still in close contact. Nine chromosomes arrange themselves in the equatorial plate for division in the one type of secondary spermatocyte (Fig. 31), and nine plus the sex-chromosome in the other (Fig. 30). All of the chromosomes, including the sex-element when it is present, divide in these cells (Figs. 32-38). 4. Spermatids. The division of the secondary spermatocyte gives rise in the one case to spermatids containing nine chromosomes (Figs. 36- 38), and in the other case nine plus the sex-chromosome or ten (Figs. 33 and 34). All of the chromosomes except the sex- chromosome are bivalent in nature so that in reality we have the equivalent of eighteen chromosomes in the one kind of spermatid and eighteen plus the sex-chromosome in the other. The bivalent chromosomes frequently begin to separate before the division of the cell is complete. Occasionally the eighteen chromosomes can be distinguished as independent elements after the cell divides (Fig. 38), although the chromosomes usually disintegrate before complete separation can be identified. All of the foregoing evidence indicates that eighteen is the reduced number of chro- mosomes. V. DIMORPHISM IN THE SPERMATOZOA. The spermatozoa of the bull vary considerably in size, and careful measurements show that they may be arranged in two separate classes, one type being much larger than the other. Mature specimens, which were free in the lumen of the tubule and parallel to the objective, were selected at random from a single slide and outline sketches of six hundred heads were made with the aid of a camera lucida. The lengths of the sketches were then carefully measured and recorded in quarter millimeters. Figure I in the text shows the variation in size of the six hundred 298 J. E. WODSEDALEK. heads measured. It shows a distinct bimodal curve with modes at 12.50 mm., and 14.75 mm. The intermodal depression is . 50 IV. 75 FIG. i. Diagram showing the variation in size among six hundred mature spermatozoa of the bull. deep and wide and the two elements of the curve are approxi- mately equal as regards number of individuals. The spermato- zoa of the larger type undoubtedly possess the sex-chromosome, while those of the smaller type are without it. Figures 39 and 40 show the comparative size of the two distinct types of mature spermatozoa. They can be distinguished with ease under a high-power microscope. The general scheme of the development of the spermatozoon from the spermatid of the bull is similar to that in the pig and the horse (Wodsedalek '13 and '14) and, therefore, will not be described here. Bimodal curves were also shown by the writer in connection with the spermatozoa of the pig and the horse, and in a number of other species by Zeleny and Faust ('14 and '15) and by Zeleny and Senay ('15). VI. OOGONIA. The best ovarian material studied in connection with the number of chromosomes in the oogonia was obtained from some of the small foetuses. The results obtained from this material STUDIES ON THE CELLS OF CATTLE. were corroborated in the tissue obtained from cows and heifers, although the adult tissue was not nearly as satisfactory. Most excellent material was found in the ovaries of a four and a half inch fcetus and one six-inch fcetus. These were fixed in the Benin's plus chromic acid fixing fluid. The oogonia were ap- parently in extreme activity at the time of fixation. Mitotic figures are abundant and the chromosomes are very distinct, especially in the late prophase and early metaphase stages which are numerous (Figs. 44-46). In general these cells resemble the last spermatogonial cells (compare Figs. 44 and 10), except that the oogonia are somewhat larger. In well fixed and favorably stained material the oogonia in the resting stage invariably show two large nucleoli each corre- sponding to the single nucleolus of the resting stage of spermato- gonial cells (Fig. 41). These retain their individuality during the spireme stage though at times they are somewhat distended (Fig. 42). When the spireme breaks up the chromosomes are long and narrow and variously curved (Fig. 43). In the late prophase they become shorter and thicker and appear evenly distributed throughout the large nuclei (Figs. 44 and 45). In this stage as well as in the early metaphase stages of division (Fig. 46) hundreds of accurate counts were possible. The two sex-chromosomes can be easily distinguished in all of the prophase stages (Figs. 41-45). Thirty-eight chromosomes are present in the oogonia (Figs. 43-46). Thirty-six are the ordinary chromosomes corresponding to the thirty-six ordinary chromosomes in the spermatogonia of the male. The two other elements are the sex-chromosomes. When the chromosomes arrange themselves in the equatorial plate for division the sex-chromosomes are always at the periphery (Figs. 46 and 47). During division all of the chromo- somes, including the sex-elements, divide in two (Fig. 48). All of the figures of the oogonial cells were made from the same section (Figs. 41-48). On account of the great significance of the two sex-chromo- somes in the femal tissue a tremenduous amount of time was devoted to this particular phase of the problem; this was also true of the studies in connection with the chromosomes of the 3OO J. E. WODSEDALEK. somatic tissue in both sexes. The final preparation of the paper for publication was postponed on several occasions, not at all because of any uncertainties, since the results were convincing from the start, but rather because each additional survey of the entire problem from new material proved more fascinating and gratifying than those preceding. VII. CHROMOSOMES IN SOMATIC CELLS. Numerous slides were made of various somatic structures from foetuses of both sexes. The organs most frequently used were the brain, lung, liver, Wolffian body, kidney, and intestine. The larger embryos were cut into pieces all of which were then sectioned. Three small embryos, ranging from ten to fourteen millimeter neck-lengths were sectioned in toto. While splendid mitotic figures and late prophase stages were found in many parts of the embryos the very best cells were found in the brain. In the larger specimens the liver and kidneys showed the most favorable cells. The male somatic cells, like the spermatogonia, contain thirty- seven chromosomes, of which thirty-six are the ordinary chromo- somes and one is the accessory element or sex-chromosome (Figs. 51 and 52). The female somatic cells, like the oogonia, contain thirty-eight chromosomes, of which thirty-six are the ordinary chromosomes and the other two are the sex-elements (Figs. 49 and 50). The sex-chromosomes in each case were as distinguish- able as they are in the germinal cells. Literally thousands of somatic cells were carefully studied in each sex. After the chromosomes of the first one hundred of the most favorable male somatic cells were studied and carefully sketched it was found that in ninety-three cases there were thirty-six of the ordinary chromosomes present plus the one accessory. In the other seven cases there were slight discrepancies, usually one or two less. This was in all probability due to unnoticeable overlapping. In two cases there were two extra chromosomes present. This was in all probability due to the fact that two of the chromosomes had divided, since these cells were in the early metaphase stage. This interpretation appears to be correct, since it was found later that occasionally some of the chromo- STUDIES ON THE CELLS OF CATTLE. 3OI somes divide considerably in advance of the others. This point was observed in several of the early metaphase stages; it was fairly common in polar views of late metaphase stages, and in a few instances a chromosome was found almost completely split in two even in the late prophase. For this reason, in selecting cells for accurate chromosome counts great care must be exercised not to select polar views of late metaphase stages. The increase in number may also be due to fragmentation, although one would expect this only in poorly fixed material or general poor technique. After the counts in the first one hundred camera-lucida sketches showed that over ninety per cent, of the cells contained thirty- seven chromosomes, no more sketches were made. However, hundreds of other counts were made from favorable cells off-hand with about the same results. This shows that in at least ninety per cent, of the male somatic cells thirty-six ordinary chromo- somes, and a single sex-chromosome were present. The ten per cent, of discrepancies is undoubtedly due to unnatural states and probable sources of error. The studies of the chromosomes in the somatic cells of the female were conducted in the same manner. Among the first one hundred cells, ninety-one showed thirty-six ordinary chromosomes and two accessories. In several hundred further counts, the percentage of discrepancies was about the same. In the case of the six small embryos in which sex could not be determined morphologically, or was uncertain, one was poorly fixed, although it was cut into small pieces, and, therefore, could not be used in this study. Of the other five two were unquestion- ably female and three were male, according to evidences from the cytological standpoint. In the case of the two specimens, cells in various parts of the body repeatedly showed thirty-six ordinary chromosomes and two accessories. In many instances where the ordinary chromosomes could not be counted, the two sex chromo- somes were recognizable. In the other three specimens the cells invariably showed only a single sex-chromosome, and in many cases the thirty-six ordinary chromosomes were counted. These extensive studies indicate quite conclusively that thirty- six is the number of ordinary chromosomes in the somatic cells of both sexes and that the male cells contain one sex-chromosome 302 J. E. WODSEDALEK. while the female cells contain two, making a total of thirty-seven in the male and thirty-eight in the female. These numbers correspond exactly with those of the spermatogonia in the male and the oogonia in the female. This is very significant in relation to our chromosome theory of sex-determination. VIII. SEX-CHROMOSOMES IN RELATION TO SEX-DETERMINATION. It was shown that in the process of spermatogenesis two distinct types of spermatozoa are produced of exactly the same number. The one type contains eighteen ordinary chromosomes plus one Sex-chromosome, and the other type contains only the eighteen ordinary chromosomes; this being the result of the unequal primary spermatocyte division, where the eighteen bivalent chromosomes divide and the unpaired sex-chromosome passes over to one pole undivided. In the oogonia there are thirty-six ordinary chromosomes plus two sex-chromosomes. Before the reduction division of the primary oocytes, in all probability (though this was not actually determined in this animal), all of the chromosomes, including the two sex-chromosomes, pair. This eventually gives rise to ova all of which contain the reduced number of chromosomes or eighteen ordinary chromosomes plus one sex-chromosome. Since the two types of spermatozoa are produced in equal numbers, fertilization by the one kind or the other is equally possible, and the number of male and female calves born is about equal if a fairly large number of offspring is considered. Sex in the offspring, as determined at the time of fertilization of the ovum by the one or the other type of spermatozoon may be illustrated as follows: Spermatozoa. Ova. Offspring. (18 + i) + (18 + i) = (36 + 2) = female, (18 + o) + (18 + i) = (36 + i) = male. The results of the above combinations are in exact accord with the number of chromosomes found in the germinal and somatic cells of the two sexes in cattle; and the relation of the sex-chromosomes to sex-determination can not be doubted. All of the five hundred or more so-called theories or rules for con- STUDIES ON THE CELLS OF CATTLE. 303 trolling sex, including the one in relation to time of service which is commonly practiced by animal breeders, must be abandoned. There is considerable literature on the metabolic theories of sex-determination which will not be discussed here. However, reference may be made to the brief though able discussion of this subject by Babcock and Clausen ('18). IX. SEX RATIO IN CATTLE. It might be well to quote here Pearl's ('17) more recent results of extensive studies on the control of the sex ratio in cattle. He says: "Some earlier statistics appeared to indicate that there was a possibility of influencing the sex ratio by paying attention to this point. It was believed to be of such extreme importance as to justify the careful study of the matter on the basis of much more extended statistics. These statistics we have now collected and analyzed and shall publish as soon as possible. In the mean- time it may be reported that, with the more extended statistics in hand, it appears to be conclusively established that there is no definite or permanent relation between the time in the heat period at which the cow is served and the sex of the offspring. The apparent relation between these two factors, which is believed by many breeders to exist and which our earlier statistics ap- peared to indicate, seems now to be purely accidental, and to have arisen only because of the comparative meagerness of the statistics on which the matter was discussed. TABLE II. SHOWING THE SEX OF THE CALVES FOLLOWING SERVICE AT DIFFERENT PARTS OF THE HEAT PERIOD. Heat Period. Lapsed Time in Hours from Ap- pearance of Heat to Service. Sex of Offspring. Per Cent, of Males. Males. Females. Early . Under 3 hours Over 3 and under 8 hours Over 8 hours 2OO 270 I87 IQ2 252 212 51.0 51-7 46.9 Middle Late Totals 657 656 50.0 'The summarized results of 1 ,313 separate and distinct matings given in Table II. will demonstrate this point. In each one of these 1,313 cases the following facts were accurately known, and 304 J. E. WODSEDALEK. reported in such a way that any bias, conscious, or unconscious, of the observer could not have influenced the result: (a) the time in hours from the first appearance of heat (oestrum), as noticed by the breeder, to the time the cow was successfully served; (&) the sex of the calf resulting from this service. "It is evident from this table that there is no significant preponderance of females when service is early in heat. There is not now known any method by which the sex ratio or propor- tion of the sexes in cattle may be effectively controlled by the breeder. A more detailed account of the results, together with further statistics will be published elsewhere." X. SEX-LIMITED INHERITANCE IN CATTLE. The discovery of the remarkable behavior of certain characters in heredity which can only be plausibly explained by supposing that they are linked with a sex-chromosome or a sex-determining factor still further strengthens our belief in the existence of such a definite factor. Wentworth ('16) reported a case which seems to fall under this general sex-limited group in the inheritance of black-and-white in Ayrshire cattle. While the general breed color is red-and-white, black-and-white animals have been known for some time, as shown by Kuhlman ('15). It is difficult to state whether the black is due to a true black pigment or whether it is simply a very dense red, since chemical solutions have not yet been attempted. In summarizing the results of the different crosses, Wentworth says, "If the factor of the black-and-white color is represented by B, the hereditary constitutions are as follows: BB is always black-and-white; bb is always red-and-white; Bb is always black-and-white in the male and red-and-white in the female. All of the nine possible matings were discovered, as shown in Table I. "The expectations here presented are based on the most probable result of each of the matings, considered on an indi- vidual basis with reference to the number of animals produced by each type of mating, but without figuring the proportions of the sexes as equal. From these data it would appear that the black-and-white color of Ayrshire cattle behaves in an ordinary STUDIES ON THE CELLS OF CATTLE. 305 sex-limited manner similar to the horns in sheep as discussed by Wood ('05) and the rudimentary mammae in swine as reported by Wentworth ('16)." TABLE I. RESULTS OF NINE POSSIBLE MATINGS OF AYRSHIRE CATTLE. Sires. Dams. Male Offspring. Female Offspring. Black-and- white. Red-and-white. B'ack-and- white. Red-and-wWite. BB BB I O IO 3 I 4 o 2 0 0 O O 0 o 5 0 I 7 3 o o 2 I O o 0 0 0 I IO I O 4 3 2 9 BB Bb. . . BB. bb. Bb. . . . BB Bb Bb Bb bb bb BB bb Bb. . . bb bb. . Total . 21 20.7^ 13 I^.2S 6 =;.2<; 30 ^0.7^ Expected. . The simple Mendelian scheme of inheritance is quite common in cattle but, to my knowledge, this is the only case of sex-limited inheritance reported in this animal ; nevertheless, it is significant, especially in view of our knowledge of the relation between this scheme of inheritance and the sex-chromosomes in many other species. XI. THE FREE-MARTIN. The case of the free-martin, the female of two-sexed twins in cattle, is well known to animal breeders to be perfectly sterile although rarely such females are perfectly normal. I do not wish to enter upon a discussion of this subject here for it is a big problem in itself. However, I wish to call the attention of animal husbandmen, who are not in position to keep in touch with all of the zoological literature, to the extensive and most painstaking piece of research on the free-martin by Dr. Frank R. Lillie ('17). In this connection I might also call attention to his article on sex-determination and sex-differentiation in mammals (Lillie, '17). XII. SUMMARY. i. Thirty-seven chromosomes occur in the spermatogonia. One, the sex-chromosome, is distinctly larger than the others. 306 J. E. WODSEDALEK. 2. Nineteen chromosomes appear in the primary spermatocyte division, of which eighteen are bivalent and the other is the unpaired sex-chromosome. 3. In the primary spermatocyte division the heart-shaped sex-chromosome passes undivided to one pole in advance of the other chromosomes. 4. The primary spermatocyte division is evidently the reduc- tion division, giving rise to two different types of secondary spermatocytes, one with the sex-chromosome and the other lacking it. 5. The one type of secondary spermatocyte, which contains the sex-chromosome, gives rise to two spermatids, each containing the sex-chromosome and eighteen ordinary chromosomes. 6. The other type of secondary spermatocyte, which lacks the sex-chromosome, gives rise to two spermatids, each containing only the eighteen ordinary chromosomes. 7. The mature spermatozoa are of two types, equal in numbers. The one type is larger and contains the sex-chromosome. The smaller type is without the sex-chromosome. The larger type is female producing, while the smaller is male producing. 8. Thirty-eight chromosomes occur in the oogonia; two of these are the sex-chromosomes. 9. The reduced number of chromosomes in the female is, in all probability, eighteen ordinary chromosomes and one sex- chromosome which apparently occurs in all of the mature ova. 10. The somatic cells of the male contain thirty-six ordinary chromosomes and one sex-chromosome. 1 1 . The somatic cells of the female contain thirty-six ordinary chromosomes and two sex-chromosomes. 12. The number of chromosomes in the somatic cells of the two sexes is in exact accord with expectations. 13. There is no relation between the time in the heat period at which the cow is served and the sex of the offspring. 14. Sex in cattle, for the present, remains a matter beyond the control of the breeder. It is determined by the sex-chromosomes ; it is a matter of inheritance. 15. Sex-limited inheritance strengthens the belief in the chro- mosome theory of sex-determination. STUDIES ON THE CELLS OF CATTLE. 307 LITERATURE CITED. Allen, Ezra. '18 Studies on Cell Division in the Albino Rat (Mus norivegicus albinus). Jour. Morph., Vol. 31, No. i. Babcock and Clausen. '18 Genetics in Relation to Agriculture. New York. Guyer, M. F. '10 Accessory Chromosomes in Man. BIOL. BULL., Vol. XIX., No. 4. Hance, R. T. '17 The Diploid Chromosome Complexes of the Pig (Siis scrofa) and their Variations. Jour. Morph., Vol. 30, No. i. Kingery, H. M. '17 Oogenesis in the White Mouse. Jour. Morph., Vol. 30, No. i. Kuhlman, A. H. '15 Black and White Ayrshires. Jour. Heredity, Vol. 6, No. 7. Lillie, F. R. '17 The Free-Martin; a Study of the Action of Sex Hormones in the Foetal Life of Cattle. Jour. Exp. Zool., Vol. 23, No. 2. '17 Sex-Determination and Sex-Differentiation in Mammals. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci., Vol. 3, pp. 464-370. Pearl, R. and Parshley, H. M. '13 Data on Sex Determination in Cattle. BIOL. BULL., Vol. 24, No. 4. Pearl, R. '17 Maine Agricultural Experiment Station Bulletin, Bulletin No. 261, Part 3, The Control of the Sex Ratio, pp. 130-131. Pratt, B. H. and Long, J. A. '17 The Period of Synapsis in the Egg of the White Rat, Mus Norwegicus Albinus. Jour. Morph., Vol. 29, No. 2. Wentworth, E. N. '16 Rudimentary Mammae in Swine a Sex-Limited Character. Science, N. S., Vol. XLIIL, No. 1114. '16 A Sex-Limited Color in Ayrshire Cattle. Jour. Agr. Research, Vol. VI., No. 4. Wodsedalek, J. E '13 Spermatogenesis of the Pig, with Special Reference to the Accessory Chromosomes. BIOL. BULL., Vol. XXV., No. i . '14 Spermatogenesis of the Horse with Special Reference to the Accessory Chromosome and the Chromatoid Body. BIOL. BULL., Vol. XXVII., No. 6. '16 Causes of Sterility in the Mule. BIOL. BULL., Vol. XXX, No. i. Wood, T. B. '05 Note on the Inheritance of Horns and Face Color in Sheep. Jour. Agr. Sci., V. i, pt. 3. Zeleny, C. and Faust, E. C. '14 Size Differences in Spermatozoa from Single Testes. Science, N . S., Vol. 39, No. 1003. '153 Size Dimorphism in the Spermatozoa from Single Testes. Jour. Exp. Zool., Vol. 18. '150 Dimorphism in Size of Spermatozoa and its Relation to the Chrom osomes Proc. Nat. Acad. Sc., Vol. i. 308 J. E. WODSEDALEK. Zeleny, C. and Senay, C. T. '15 Variation in Head Length of Spermatozoa in Seven Additional Species of Insects. Jour. Exp. Zool., Vol. 19, No. 4. EXPLANATION OF PLATES. PLATE I. (All of the drawings were made with the aid of a camera lucida, X 2,400, except Figs. 39 and 40 which are X 2,200.) FIG. i. Polar view of the metaphase of division in a spermatogonial cell show- ing thirty-six ordinary chromosomes and the single heart-shaped sex-chromosome. FIG. 2. Side view of the metaphase of division in a spermatogonial cell showing the thirty-six ordinary chromosomes, and the sex-chromosome at the left. FIGS. 3 AND 4. Late anaphases of division of spermatogonial cells showing the division of all of the chromosomes, including the large sex-chromosome. FIGS. 5-9. Late prophase stages of the last spermatogonial cells. BIOLOGICAL BULLETIN, VOL. XXXVIII. J. F. WODSEDALEK. 31 0 J. E. WODSEDALEK. PLATE II. FIGS. 10 AND ii. Last spermatogonial cells showing thirty-six ordinary chromo- somes and the large sex-chromosome. FIG. 12. Last spermatogonial cell taken from stale tissue showing the per- sistence of the segregation of the chromosomes in this stage. FIG. 13. Spireme stage of the primary spermatocyte showing the large sex- chromosome. FIGS. 14-18. Polar views of metaphase stages of division of the primary spermatocytes showing eighteen bivalent chromosomes and the sex-chromosome which is usually at the periphery of the plate. BIOLOGICAL BULLETIN, VOL.. XXXVlll. Hffitet 10 II 12 16 J. E. WODSEOALEK. \. 18' ;':": 'v :*jm .-/ v^, i -' ---:^ 312 J. E. WODSEDALEK. PLATE III. FIGS. 19-25. Metaphase stages of division of primary spermatocytes showing the passing of the sex-chromosome to one pole in advance of the other chromosomes. FIGS. 26 AND 27. Late anaphase stages of division of primary spermatocytes showing eighteen chromosomes at one pole and eighteen plus the sex-chromosome at the other. BIOLOGICAL BULLETIN. VOL. XXXVIII. PLATE III. 20 21 22 24 26 £r?»ix ll 27 J. E. WODSEDALEK. 314 J. E. WODSEDALEK. PLATE IV. FIGS. 28 AXD 29. Cells showing the formation of the secondary spermatocytes from the primary spermatocyte division. The eighteen chromosomes pair loosely to form nine bivalents which are present in the one type of secondary spermatocyte and in the other type are shown the nine bivalents and the sex-chromosome. FIG. 30. Metaphase stage of division of the one type of secondary spermatocyte showing the nine bivalent chromosomes and the sex-chromosome. FIG. 31. Metaphase stage of division of the other type of secondary spermato- cyte showing only the nine bivalent chromosomes. FIGS. 32-34. Division stages of the secondary spermatocyte with the sex- chromosome showing the division of all of the chromosomes, including the sex- element, giving rise to spermatids both of which contain the sex-chromosome. When the division of the cell is complete (Fig. 34) each of the nine ordinary chromo- some splits into two so that in reality there are eighteen ordinary chromosomes plus the sex-chromosome in this type of spermatid. FIGS. 35-37. Division stages of the secondary spermatocyte without the sex- chromosome. Figure 37 shows the splitting up of the nine chromosomes at the poles so that there are in reality eighteen chromosomes passed on to the other type of spermatid. FIG. 38. A newly formed spermatid showing the eighteen chromosomes. FIG. 39. A mature spermatozoan of the smaller type undoubtedly without the sex-chromosome. FIG. 40. A mature spermatozoan of the larger type which undoubtedly contains the sex-chromosome. BIOLOGICAL BULLETIN, VOL XXXVIII. PLATE IV. 2S 3° 32 34 36 J t. AODSEDALEK m 33 ...... ••*.... 37 /:' • 'v^";\ /°' ,;'s>. /v /I ^:." Si 39 *,."•>. i 31 6 J. E. WODSEDALEK. PLATE V. FIG. 41. Resting stage of an oogonial cell showing two large nucleoli which are undoubtedly the sex-chromosomes. FIG. 42. Spireme stage of an oogonial cell showing the two large sex-chromo- somes. FIG. 43. An oogonial cell showing thirty-six newly formed chromosomes and the two large sex-chromosomes. FIGS. 44 AND 45. Late prophase stages of oogonial cells showing thirty-six ordinary chromosomes and the two sex-chromosomes. FIGS. 46 AND 47. Metaphase stages of division of the oogonia showing the two sex-chromosomes at the periphery of the plate. FIG. 49. Liver cell in late prophase stage taken from a female foetus showing thirty-six ordinary chromosomes and the two sex-hromosomes. FIG. 50. Brain cell in metaphase stage of division taken from a female embryo showing thirty-six ordinary chromosomes and the two sex-chromosomes. FIG. 51. Brain cell in metaphase stage of division taken from a male embryo showing thirty-six ordinary chromosomes and only one sex-chromosome. FIG. 52. Liver cell in late prophase stage taken from a male foetus showing thirty-six ordinary chromosomes and only one sex-chromosome. BIOLOGICAL BULLETIN, VOL. XXXVIII. PLATE V. 41 ''•'•• .•"•/•"•' PS&Jlt W; r-- !->;•;, •;c •:":•>; -^sf*;. •••• • 42 ";" • - .. i-c"" - , 45 49 - % 48 >,* ' J. E WODSEDALEK. STUDIES IN ARTIFICIAL PARTHENOGENESIS. III. CORTICAL CHANGE AND THE INITIATION OF MATURATION IN THE EGG OF CUMINGIA.1 L. V. HEILBRUNN This study is a record of experiments performed during the summer of 1916 at the Woods Hole Marine Biological Laboratory. As Morgan pointed out in 1910, the egg of the lamellibranch Cumingia is very suitable for study. Like most other eggs it is still immature when shed into the sea-water. Although the first maturation spindle has formed, no polar bodies are thrown off unless the egg is fertilized or treated with the proper reagents. Doubtless some change is necessary before the egg can throw off polar bodies and begin its development. An effort has been made to determine the nature of this change. Many diverse reagents cause the egg to ma-ture. Al- though all of these reagents do not occasion the same morpho- logical transformations nevertheless all of them agree in having one specific physical effect on the egg. All release the egg cyto- plasm from the restraint of a rigid enveloping membrane. The immature unfertilized egg is surrounded by a stiff vitelline membrane which presses tightly in on it and effectively prevents the throwing off of polar bodies. It is only when the egg is released from this restraint that maturation can proceed. PHYSICAL MAKE-UP OF THE EGG. As in Arbacia, the Cumingia egg is a mass of fluid protoplasm, surrounded by a rigid membrane. Only a few turns of the centrifuge are sufficient to throw to opposite poles of the egg the substances suspended in the cytoplasm. To one pole pass the presumably lighter oil globules, to the opposite pole the heavier pigment. But these suspended particles can go no farther than the poles, for there they are stopped by the vitelline membrane which surrounds the egg. This is a stiff structure and is easily 1 Contribution from the Zoological Laboratory, University of Michigan, New Series, no. 3. 317 31 8 L. V. HEILBRUNN. visible under higher power. It is about one micron in thickness. The vitelline membrane must be thought of as the plasma mem- brane of the egg. As I have pointed out before, students of cellular mechanics have been blind to the fact that egg cells are provided with visible membranes which are plasma-mem- branes. They have often insisted that no one has ever seen a plasma-membrane. It is easy to prove that the vitelline mem- membrane of the Cumingia egg governs osmotic intercourse and is therefore a plasma-membrane. When Cumingia eggs are put into hypertonic solutions they shrink only very slightly. The stiffness of the vitelline plasma- membrane prevents a marked shrinkage. Moreover, a weakly hypertonic solution produces just as much shrinkage as a strong one provided that it does not alter the membrane. On the other hand if the hypertonic solution makes the membrane less rigid by causing it to swell, then the egg shrinks to a great extent. Facts such as these can be interpreted only on the assumption of a stiff plasma-membrane. That this is the vitelline membrane is certain, for there is no other membrane in the vicinity. Cer- tainly there is none inside the vitelline membrane, for when the egg has been left for some time in a hypertonic solution of con- siderable strength then the coagulated cytoplasm shrinks away from the vitelline membrane and presents a rough uneven sur- face. Under these conditions it is obviously not surrounded by any membrane. As to the chemical nature of the plasma-membrane it is essentially protein. It swells in dilute acids or alkalies, and in sodium chloride or sodium iodide solutions. Moreover it does not contain any large admixture of lipoid as can be shown by test- ing it with Scharlach R solution. Like many other marine eggs the Cumingia egg is surrounded by a diffuse jelly of the same refractive index as sea-water and therefore invisible. It can easily be demonstrated by various vital stains (e.g., Nile blue sulphate) or by India ink suspensions. This jelly has no apparent effect on the early developmental phenomena. If the eggs are shaken a few times in a test-tube, they are deprived of their jelly. Such eggs react in the same way as those with jelly intact. STUDIES IN ARTIFICIAL PARTHENOGENESIS. 319 As in Arbacia the Cumin gia vitelline membrane is capable of two kinds of cortical change. Both membrane elevation and membrane swelling can occur. If the egg is placed in solutions of low surface tension the tension of the membrane is lowered and it rapidly lifts away from the egg. No doubt the explanation of this process is the same as that I have offered for the similar process in the sea-urchin egg. But in Cumingia membrane elevation is not the normal cortical change. When eggs are inseminated the membrane does not become elevated, it becomes swollen. Treatment of eggs with a reagent which causes either mem- brane elevation or membrane swelling will result in a throwing off of polar bodies. Thus just as in the sea-urchin egg either swelling or elevation provides the necessary cortical change, although in this egg it is swelling and not elevation which is normal. Finally there is a third way in which maturation can be initiated. This consists in the removal of the membrane. I shall now proceed to the experimental data considering first membrane elevation, second membrane swelling, and third the removal of the membrane. MEMBRANE ELEVATION. Any substance which markedly lowers the surface tension of sea-water is effective in producing membrane elevation. But if elevation is to be followed by maturation only certain concentra- tions and certain lengths of exposure can be used in each case. Too high a concentration or too long an exposure generally leads to coagulation. Frequently an over-exposure produces a rupture of the egg membrane, the protoplasm then flows out to form an exovate. Such eggs soon disintegrate. Numerous reagents were used to produce a lowering of surface tension. Of course the number of successful reagents could have been increased many fold. Thus many of the higher alcohols no doubt act in the same way as the amyl alcohol which I used. In the experiments the same general proceedure was always employed. In every case, the eggs of only a single female were used. Owing to the fact that the number of eggs obtainable from a single female is comparatively small, I found it best to 32O L. V. HEILBRUNN. perform all of the experiments in low Stender dishes (containing about 50 cc.) instead of in larger fingerbowls. These dishes were very convenient, for they could without any trouble be put on the stage of the microscope. One Stender dish was made to contain the desired reagent and then the eggs were pipetted into it- After varying lengths of exposure the eggs were transferred from the reagent to fresh sea-water also contained in Stender dishes. Then after twenty minutes or more had elapsed the treated eggs were examined. As the polar bodies of Cumingia are unusually large it is possible to count the percentage of matured eggs under low power and without removing the eggs to a slide. Counts were usually made in this way. Obviously it is not possible to see the polar bodies if the egg is lying with the animal pole down. Hence the per cent, recorded is always too low. Even if all the eggs had polar bodies not many more than fifty per cent, would show them. If the eggs are turned about this difficulty can of course be ob- viated, but turning is a tedious process and was only occasionally resorted to. Thus the counts recorded represent minima and are really only about half as high as they should be. Counts are given in the form of fractions, in which the denominator repre- sents the total number of eggs counted the numerator the number of eggs with polar bodies. For example the fraction 10/50 would indicate that out of 50 eggs counted 10 showed a polar body. In making up per cent, solutions of volatile liquids, it was found convenient to use 100 c.c. measuring flasks. Thus if a 2 per cent, ether solution was desired 2 c.c. of ether was placed in a 100 c.c. measuring flask and sea-water was added until the solution reached the 100 c.c. mark. Owing to the diminution in volume on mixing the two liquids such a solution is not exactly a 2 per cent. one. However, after the above method of procedure it is easier to calculate the molecular concentration. The accompanying table gives the results of experiments with eleven substances which lower surface tension. For each sub- stance used it was necessary first to determine the proper con- centration. Sometimes this was a simple matter. Thus for saponin almost any concentration is successful. But usually only a narrow range of concentrations will produce the desired STUDIES IN ARTIFICIAL PARTHENOGENESIS. 321 effect. If the reagent is just a little too strong it can not be effectively employed. In many cases only a certain length of exposure is suitable. Too long exposures generally produce exovates. Formation of exovates is represented by the symbol "e" in the table. With ether and chloroform it is particularly difficult to obtain maturation. In both instances I at first despaired of success. A 3 per cent, solution of ether in sea-water did not produce maturation. With such a solution exovates generally appeared. On the other hand 2.5 per cent, ether had no observable effect on the eggs. With a concentration intermediate between 2.5 per cent, and 3 per cent, success was attained. In the table are given the figures for a representative experiment with ether. In the case of chloroform the range of effective solutions is even narrower. In making up chloroform solutions very small quantities of the liquid had to be measured out. This was done by counting the drops from a small pipette which was calibrated for the purpose. About 60 drops from this pipette constituted i c.c. By placing 8 such drops into a 100 c.c. measuring flask and diluting to the mark a 0.13 per cent, solution was obtained. This solution produced but little effect upon the eggs. Exposures of 3-5 minutes showed only 1-2 per cent, of polar body formation. Thus a 0.13 per cent, solution was apparently too weak. On the other hand, a 0.17 per cent, solution proved too strong. Such a solution was prepared by diluting 15 drops of chloroform (from the pipette mentioned above) up to 100 c.c. with sea-water. A 0.25 per cent, solution was thus obtained and this was then diluted to 0.17 per cent, by adding 5 c.c. of sea-water to 10 c.c. of the solution. The resultant 0.17 per cent, solution was found to be too strong, for it produced exovates. Moreover viscosity tests with the centrifuge showed that it coagulated the egg cyto- plasm. Although neither 0.13 per cent, nor 0.17 per cent, chloroform produced maturation, a concentration slightly under 0.17 per cent. did. This solution was made by diluting 10 drops from the pipette up to 100 c.c. Such a solution would ordinarily give 0.17 per cent., but one particular solution was made on an extremely hot day (when the room temperature was 27°). No doubt the drops from the pipette were smaller, owing to the 322 L. V. HEILBRUNN. lower surface tension. Hence the concentration which resulted was slightly under 0.17 per cent. In the table I have referred to this solution as "0.16 per cent." As the figures show, it proved highly effective. TABLE I. POLAR BODY FORMATION AFTER TREATMENT WITH SUBSTANCES WHICH LOWER SURFACE TENSION. Exposure Minutes. i Per Cent. Amyl Alco- hol. 2.5 Per Cent. Ethyl Ace- tate. 0.25 Per Cent. Ethyl Buty- rate. Pe5r Cent. Ethyl Ni- trate. 2.8 Per Cent. Ether. '0.16 Per Cent.' Chloro- form. 5 Per Cent. Aceto- nitrile. Satu- rated Solution Phenyl Ure- thane. O.2 Per Cent. Chlore- tone. Emul- sion1 Toluol. O.2 Per Cent. Sa- ponin. I 0 O 0 0 O 29 12 4 SO SO 50 IOO IOO IOO 50 i o O O 0 O £3 2 0 18 3 2O 2 SO 50 SO SO IOO So 50 IOO IOO IOO 50 I i 0 o 2 I 29 12 o o 10 33 50 50 50 50 ' IOO 50 50 IOO IOO IOO 50 12 4 22 14 6 o o 2 25 50 IOO IOO IOO e e IOO IOO e 12 IO 15 o 0 33 3 1 * e e e e e 25 50 IOO IOO IOO 50 3£ 13 22 o 18 4 e e e e e 50 5°9 IOO IOO SO 19 18 I 20 5 e e e e — 50 IOO IOO 50 £4 12 232 2 6 , e 25 SO 5° IOO 15 19 7 e e e e e IOO 50 8 23 5 50 IOO 3 16 5 9 10 — e e e e 25 50 IOO IOO 15 43 » 200 All of the substances mentioned in the table produced a similar morphological change in the egg. All of them caused the vitelline membrane to become lifted away from the cytoplasm. The details of this membrane elevation were watched in a great many different experiments. The membrane first detached itself from the egg at a number of points around the periphery. As a result it appeared slightly thicker and for a minute or two it often 1 This emulsion was made by stirring i c.c. ol toluol with 3 c.c. or sea water. 2 Eggs were shaken while counting. As mentioned above this gives a higher count. STUDIES IN ARTIFICIAL PARTHENOGENESIS. 323 showed small outbulgings which gave it a more or less crenate appearance. Very soon, the membrane lifted away with con- siderable rapidity. If success was to be obtained the egg had to be removed from the solution at just about the time the crena- tions appeared. As in Arbacia when the vitelline membrane is lifted from the egg surface, a new membrane, no doubt a precipitation membrane, is immediately formed about the egg cytoplasm. Sometimes it is possible to cause this to become elevated also. Saponin produces the best results. When eggs are exposed for 40 minutes to 0.2 per cent, saponin they are found to be surrounded by two membranes. The outer one of these is quite far from the egg and it was thought at first that it might represent the outer edge of the jelly which had become visible. But this was shown not to be true, for two membranes could be produced about eggs from which the jelly had previously been shaken off. I have used the eggs of Cumingia for a number of years and in some years I have found a tendency for a small per cent, of the eggs to mature without apparently any treatment. In the experi- ments recorded in this paper careful controls were always kept. In every case at least two hundred eggs of the untreated- control were examined for polar body formation. Of the experiments cited in the table, in only one instance did the control show any maturation. In the control for the ethyl nitrate experiment one egg showed a polar body out of considerably over two hundred examined. The table shows that eleven substances can produce polar body formation and this is in every case preceeded by membrane elevation. These eleven substances differ from each other very widely in chemical constitution. It is almost inconceivable that they should have any one chemical effect in common. Their action must be primarily physical. It is believed to involve a lowering of surface tension. The explanation which I have offered for membrane elevation in Arbacia applies equally well for Cumingia. For details of this explanation the reader is referred to my earlier papers. Of course by choosing substances similar to those listed in the table numerous other successful reagents could no doubt be 324 L. V. HEILBRUNN. discovered. Thus it is probable that benzol or xylol would behave as toluol, and that many other alcohols and esters could have been added to those given in the table. Such experiments would scarcely add material for the general argument. On the other hand it might be thought that the eleven sub- stances in the table represent only a few out of the many that I have tried. It perhaps not infrequently happens that experi- menters suppress the record of their failures. But in these experiments practically every substance selected because of its effect on surface tension gave the expected result. There were only three exceptions and in two of these cases I performed only a single experiment with a single concentration of the reagent. Moreover in each of these three cases the reagent employed had some secondary effect on the egg. I shall consider each case in detail. Ethyl Urethane. — 3 per cent, ethyl urethane had practically no effect, although in longer exposures (5-7 minutes) a few eggs with polar bodies were observed. The reagent has some action on the jelly that I have not analyzed. Possibly it is a shrinkage effect. Two minutes after the egg entered the solution it was surrounded by queer looking bubbles. Fifteen minutes later the bubbles had disappeared and their place had apparently been taken by a zone of radiating lines. Nitromethane. — I used a 5 per cent, solution of nitromethane and exposed the eggs to it for intervals of from ^-10 minutes. No polar bodies were produced as a result of the treatment. Exposure for 4 minutes followed by transfer to normal sea-water resulted in rupture of the vitelline membrane and disintegration of the egg. The eggs left in the nitromethane solution showed a queer transformation. They lost their spherical shape and flattened out into discs resembling huge red blood corpuscles. In addition to this queer effect the reagent also appeared to have some action on the jelly, for some morphological changes were visible around the egg. The nature of these changes I did not stop to investigate. Acetone. — Two concentrations of acetone were tried. 25 per cent, acetone did not produce membrane elevation when eggs were exposed i-n minutes. 50 per cent, acetone produced STUDIES IN ARTIFICIAL PARTHENOGENESIS. 325 membrane elevation, but not polar body formation. Of the eggs exposed \ minute, 72 per cent, had widely elevated mem- branes. These eggs, however, could not produce polar bodies for they were thoroughly coagulated. This was shown by a viscosity test with the centrifuge. MEMBRANE SWELLING. If instead of being lifted off, the vitelline membrane is made to swell, much the same effect is produced on the egg. The increased fluidity of the vitelline membrane results in a lower surface tension.1 Consequently it no longer exerts as great a pressure upon the egg contents. Thus maturation follows mem- brane swelling just as it follows membrane elevation. In order to produce a swelling of the membrane the same reagents were used that were previously found to have been effective for the sea-urchin egg. Evidently the vitelline mem- branes of both Cumingia and Arbacia are similar, for they swell under the same conditions. Sodium Iodide. — Eggs were exposed to 0.6 M sodium iodide. After 15 and 22^ minutes they were removed to sea-water in Stender dishes A and B respectively. Of the eggs in A, 14/100 showed polar bodies. The eggs in B formed no polar bodies. The sodium iodide solution caused membrane swelling. Hydrochloric Acid. — Eggs were placed in 25 c.c. of sea-water plus 0.7 c.c. n/io HC1. After exposures of |, i, 2, 3, 4, 5, 7, 10 minutes the eggs were removed from the acid solution and placed in ordinary sea-water in Stender dishes A— H respectively. In the acidified sea-water the egg membrane swelled and the surface of the egg became sticky. Often the eggs adhered to the bottom of the dish. Counts of eggs with polar bodies gave the following results: A }<2 minute exposure 6/50 B i 5/50 C i 10/50 D 3 4/50 (This count was made too early) E 4 13/50 F 5 IS/50 G 7 10/50 H 10 8/50 1 Many biologists apparently do not understand that solids and pseudo-solids (i.e., gels) exhibit surface tension. This tension is greater for a gel than for the corresponding sol. For references to literature on this subject consult Heilbrunn '15, footnote, p. 166. 326 L. V. HEILBRUNN. The eggs which were allowed to remain in the acid sea-water also formed a few polar-bodies. Potassium Hydroxide. — Eggs were placed in 40 c.c. of sea-water plus i c.c. n/io KOH. In this solution membrane swelling occurred. The eggs formed polar bodies while in the alkaline medium. The first polar body observed was noted after 16 minutes exposure. After 33 minutes, a count gave 14/100 with polar bodies. Potassium Cyanide. — Eggs were placed in a 0.04 per cent. KCN solution, made by diluting 5 c.c. of 2 per cent. KCN up to 250 c.c. with sea-water. During the experiment the cyanide was not allowed to evaporate, for it was kept in a tightly stoppered weighing-tube. After a 36 minute exposure a count showed 28/100 of the eggs with polar bodies. The potassium cyanide caused membrane swelling probably because of its alka- line reaction. Hypertonic Sodium Chloride Solution. — In the experiments with acids and alkalies and with sodium iodide the solutions used were approximately isotonic with sea-water. In the course of some other work it was noticed that solutions made by adding 2\ M NaCl to sea-water caused membrane swelling. Hence it was expected that these solutions would also cause polar body formation. Eggs were exposed for 7 minutes to a solution made by adding 5 c.c. of 2| M NaCl to 25 c.c. of sea-water. As a result 21/100 of the eggs formed polar bodies. To sum up, in all of these experiments the swelling of the vitelline membrane was in every case followed by the throwing-off of polar bodies in a large percentage of the eggs. The controls of untreated eggs did not form polar bodies. In these experiments with reagents which cause membrane swelling there were no failures. No reagent could be discovered which would produce swelling of the vitelline membrane without at the same time causing the eggs to mature. RUPTURE OR REMOVAL OF THE MEMBRANE. There is a third way in which the Cumingia egg may be freed from the binding pressure of its vitelline membrane. If the eggs are shaken vigorously, oftentimes a certain percentage of the STUDIES IN ARTIFICIAL PARTHENOGENESIS. 327 membranes will be shaken off. Apparently the membrane rup- tures at the animal pole of the egg and owing to its elasticity it shrinks away toward the vegetal pole. In many cases its wrinkled remains can be found at this pole. No doubt after the membrane is shaken off a new precipitation membrane forms about the cytoplasm but this is very much less rigid than the original vitelline membrane. The results obtained from shaking Cumingia eggs are somewhat variable. The eggs must be shaken sufficiently to rupture or remove the membrane, but they must not be shaken too vigorously or too long. Too much shaking interferes with the mitotic processes underlying maturation and a smaller percentage of polar bodies results. This is in accord- ance with the observation of Wilson ('01) that shaking prevents cell division. A number of shaking experiments were performed. In one of the best of these, the eggs were placed in a small 10 c.c. test tube and shaken vigorously by swinging the forearm from a vertical to a horizontal position. They were given 40 such swings in 10 seconds. When these shaken eggs were examined an hour and a half later it was found that 27/56 had polar bodies. Thus practically all of them had matured, for as pointed out in the first part of this paper, the counts represent minima. That the shaking process had actually resulted in a removal of the membrane could be demonstrated in three ways. In the first place the remains of the membrane could often be seen at the vegetal pole. Secondly, when the polar bodies formed they did not appear to be within a stiff membrane as in polar body formation after fertilization. Lastly some shaken eggs were placed in a drop of acetone, of these only 3 out of 50 showed membrane elevation. When normal unshaken eggs were simi- larly treated with acetone all of them showed an elevated membrane. Finally there is still another method of freeing the egg from the restraint of its membrane. Although this method produces practically the same results as shaking, the procedure is very different. It was found that in diluted sea-water rupture of the vitelline membrane occurred and as was to be expected, matura- tion followed. Obviously in diluted sea-water the osmotic pres- 328 L. V. HEILBRUNN. sure forces water to enter the eggs and the membrane bursts- In many instances the ruptured membrane could be seen at one side of the egg. Sometimes exovates were produced. Various dilutions were employed, from pure distilled water to a mixture of I part of distilled water to 2 parts of sea-water. In one experiment eggs were exposed to distilled water for 40, 60 and 80 seconds and then returned to sea-water. All three exposures were successful. The 40 second exposure was not counted, the 60 second exposure showed 51/100 polar bodies and the 80 second exposure 52/100 polar bodies. The controls were normal and over 200 eggs which were counted showed no signs of maturation. All the exposures showed some signs of cleavage. One egg reached a stage with about 16 cells. In another experiment eggs were placed into 30 c.c. of sea-water plus 15 c.c. of distilled water. Of these eggs 13/50 showed polar bodies. THE SIGNIFICANCE OF CORTICAL CHANGE. It has been shown that three types of cortical change can be produced in the Cumingia egg. All of these free the egg from the restraint of a stiff vitelline membrane. This release from restraint may then be thought of as the essential feature of cortical change and as the direct cause of maturation. When it occurs maturation follows; without it no maturation takes place. It might be argued that all of the types of cortical change have some other common effect besides the one mentioned. Perhaps they all directly produce an increase of oxidations, which then causes maturation to follow. This would be a diffi- cult relation to conceive of chemically. Moreover there is experi- mental evidence that maturation does not depend on an increase of oxidations. The supporters of the oxidation theory of initiation of develop- ment have always held that dilute cyanide solutions check oxidations. Thus Loeb, '13, states on p. 26: "It has long been known that the oxidations in the cell can be prevented by the addition of a little potassium cyanide, even when oxygen is present. I have found that the addition of 0.5 c.c. of a 1/20 per cent. KCN solution to 50 c.c. of sea-water is sufficient to stop almost immediately the effect of the spermatozoon in the fertilized sea-urchin egg." Such a solution is 0.0005 Per cent. STUDIES IN ARTIFICIAL PARTHENOGENESIS. 329 Some Cumingia eggs were placed in a 0.04 per cent, solution of potassium cyanide, five minutes after they had been fertilized. The solution was prepared by diluting 5 c.c. of 2 per cent. KCN up to 250 c.c. with sea-water. Solution and eggs were kept in a glass-stoppered weighing-tube to guard against evaporation of the cyanide. Under these conditions practically all of the fertilized eggs formed the first polar body. Actual count with- out turning over the eggs, showed 24/50. On the other hand it might be thought that all types of cortical change produce an increase of permeability. In recent years various observers have claimed that the sea-urchin egg under- goes an increase in permeability either after fertilization or after artificial membrane elevation. These observers have en- deavored to show: (i) An increased penetration of dyes, (2) a drop in electrical resistance, (3) a more rapid passage of water into or out of the cell. When fertilized sea-urchin eggs are placed in dilute solutions of methylene blue, they stain more rapidly than do unfertilized eggs, according to Lyon and Shackell, '10. Runnstrom, 'n, obtained similar results with methylene blue although not with neutral red. The experiments of Lyon and Shackell are fre- quently cited and are always taken to indicate an increased permeability after fertilization.1 When unfertilized and fertilized Cumingia eggs are placed in dilute solutions of methylene blue or neutral red, the unfertilized eggs take up the dye just as rapidly as do the fertilized eggs. Fertilized and unfertilized eggs were put into Syracuse dishes containing methylene blue solutions of various strengths. From time to time the eggs were examined over a light and over a dark background, and under the microscope. The color of faintly stained eggs can be much better observed with the naked eye than with the microscope. A more accurate method of deter- mination would involve the use of a colorimeter but the method used was sufficient to show that no marked increase of permeabil- ity occurred. The dilutions of the dye were made up from a 0.5 per cent, solution of Griibler's "Methylenblau rectif. nach 1 They might however indicate nothing more than an increased affinity for dyes on the part of the cytoplasm. Such changes in staining properties are common enough, especially after changes in the colloidal state. 330 L. V. HEILBRUNN. Ehrlich." Five minutes after fertilization some fertilized eggs were placed in dishes A2 to £2 containing o.i per cent., 0.05 per cent., 0.025 per cent., 0.0125 per cent., 0.00625 per cent, methylene blue respectively. At the same time some unfertilized eggs were placed in dishes Ai to El containing similar concentra- tions of the dye. After ten minutes had elapsed, the eggs in Ai and A2 were navy blue, those in Bi and 62 pale blue, those in Ci and C2 scarcely colored and those in Di and D2, El and £2 not colored at all. Obviously there was no difference between the two sets of eggs. After fifteen minutes eggs in Ai and A2 were navy blue, those in Bi and 62 light navy blue, those in Ci and C2 light blue, the unfertilized eggs in Di were a very pale blue, the fertilized eggs in D2 uncolored, in El and £2 eggs were still uncolored. The eggs were observed at various times, but no marked changes could be observed. After an hour I thought I might be able to detect a slightly deeper color in the fertilized eggs in A2, 62, C2 than in the unfertilized eggs subjected to the same concentrations of dye in Ai, Bi, Ci, but this was probably due to the fact that the fertilized eggs were slightly more numer- ous. If entrance of stain is a test of permeability, then certainly there is no sharp difference in the permeability of fertilized and unfertilized Cumingia eggs. The concentrations of stain used in the above experiment were not injurious. Even in the most concentrated of the solutions used the eggs proceeded in their development and became motile larvse. A similar experiment was tried with neutral red. A saturated solution of the stain and 1/2, 1/4, 1/8, 1/16 saturated solutions were used. In no case could any difference in permeability between fertilized and unfertilized eggs be noted. McClendon, '10, and Gray, '16, have maintained that following fertilization there is a drop in the electrical resistance of the sea- urchin egg. They have interpreted this as indicating an increase of permeability, although of course various other explanations might be given. In the sea-urchin egg the normal process of cortical change is membrane elevation, which would interfere with the experiment. McClendon and Gray therefore were obliged either to \vait until the eggs lost their power of under- going membrane elevation or to so treat the eggs that they lost STUDIES IN ARTIFICIAL PARTHENOGENESIS. 33! this power, before they could make their measurements. In the Cumingia egg electrical measurements would be easier inas- much as the normal cortical change is not membrane elevation. But the following point should be noted. In any measure- ments of the electrical resistance of masses of egg cells, it is rather doubtful if one is measuring the resistance of the cells at all. If one conceives of a piled-up mass of spheres resting in a liquid, it is obvious after a moment's consideration that the liquid is broadly continuous from one side of the mass to the other. Thus water flows readily through a pile of shot. If now the spheres are poor conductors as compared to the liquid, and an electric current is sent through the mass, it will flow almost exclusively through the liquid. The resistance then depends on the size and shape of the interspaces between the spheres. In the case of egg cells this may vary in several ways. In the sea-urchin egg it is certain that after fertilization the interspaces between individual eggs in a mass are greater than those before fertiliza- tion, for it has been shown (cf. Heilbrunn, '15) that after fertiliza- tion the eggs offer much more resistance to compression and hence tend to preserve their spherical shape. Thus after fertiliza- tion one might expect the resistance of a mass of eggs to be lower even though the electric current did not pass through the eggs at all. In the last few years R. S. Lillie ('16, '17) has shown that when fertilized or activated eggs are placed in hypotonic solutions, water enters them more rapidly than it does unfertilized eggs. Similarly in hypertonic solutions (R. S. Lillie, '18) water leaves the fertilized eggs more readily. This is due according to Lillie to an increased permeability of the plasma membrane to water. Lillie's reasoning is a bit difficult to follow. Originally he be- lieved in an increased permeability of the membrane to salts and dissolved substances. This would of course decrease the speed of entrance of water from hypotonic solutions, or the speed of exit to hypertonic solutions, for it would decrease the osmotic pressure upon which the exit or entrance of the water depends. Osmotic pressure is, as everyone knows, dependent upon the impermeability of a membrane to dissolved substances. Increase in permeability to salts would therefore produce the opposite 332 L. V. HEILBRUNN. effect from increased permeability to water. Apparently there is a dilemma. As a matter of fact it appears to be rather far-fetched to assume a change in permeability to water, since we know the plasma membrane to be at all times permeable to it. R. Lillie's results can be much more simply explained on the basis of my conception of the plasma membrane (see Heilbrunn, '15). Nor- mally before fertilization it is a more or less rigid structure and as such resists the entrance or exit of water from the cell. I showed by measurement ('15, pp. 155-158) that when the membrane was made less rigid as a result of membrane swelling then water left the cell more readily. After fertilization the plasma membrane either itself becomes less rigid, as when membrane swelling occurs, or it is replaced by a less rigid mem- brane as a result of membrane elevation. Hence water enters and leaves the cell more rapidly. It is easy enough to decide between R. Lillie's interpretation and mine. If the difference is simply one of relative permeability to water, then in hypotonic or hypertonic solutions the water should enter or leave the fertilized eggs more rapidly, but the final equilibrium point should be the same for both fertilized and unfertilized eggs. However on the basis of my view, not only should the water enter and leave the eggs more rapidly, but the actual equilibrium state should be altered. In hypertonic solu- tions, more water should leave the fertilized eggs and in hypotonic solutions more water should enter them. In the case of hypo- tonic solutions Lillie's own figures seem to show that at equilib- rium more water has entered the fertilized eggs than the unfertilized.1 1 Lillie's measurements were made on the egg of the sea-urchin Arbacia. In this egg the presence of the elevated vitelline membrane or fertilization membrane introduces a complication. As is well known this membrane is a stiff structure. When fertilized sea-urchin eggs are subjected to hypotonic solutions they increase in size rapidly until they reach the elevated membrane. Then further increase in diameter is dependent on the power of the eggs to stretch or rupture the membrane. R. S. Lillie does not state which occurs. As a matter of fact these experiments on endosmosis were done in September when the Arbacia season is practically over. At this time the normal membrane elevation is difficult to obtain, and usually, unless the sperm concentration falls within certain very narrow limits, the membrane swells at fertilization. For eggs with swollen membranes the experiment is uncomplicated, as in Cumingia. STUDIES IN ARTIFICIAL PARTHENOGENESIS. 333 My experiments with Cumingia eggs show clearly that after fertilization not only do the eggs swell more rapidly in hypotonic solutions but their total imbitition of water is greater. Five minutes after fertilization some eggs were placed in 25 c.c. sea-water plus 15 c.c. distilled water. At the same time some unfertilized eggs were placed in a similar solution. Both sets of eggs were measured after about 45 minutes. By this time the eggs had reached an osmotic equilibrium for a second set of measurements 45 minutes later showed no further change. The results are given in Table II. TABLE II. DIAMETERS OF FERTILIZED AND UNFERTILIZED EGGS IN 25 c.c. SEA-WATER + 15 c.c. DISTILLED WATER. Average Diameter of 12 Unfertilized Eggs in Sea-water 62.65 p.. Fertilized Eggs After 40-45 Min. Fertilized Eggs After 85-90 Min. Unfertilized Eggs After 47-55 Min. Unfertilized Eggs After 92-100 Min. M M M M 70.4 69.2 66.9 67-5 69.2 69.8 66.9 67-5 73.9 X 66.9 68.7 66.9 66.9 71.0 X 68.1 69.2 68.1 66.4 69.8 X 66.9 69.2 66.9 68.1 69.8 71.5 X 63.6 68.1 67-5 69.2 X 68.7 71.5 67-5 66.9 68.1 70.4 66.9 67-5 71.0 X 67.5 69.2 66.4 68.7 70.4 70.4 68.1 68.1 72.7 69.8 68.7 67-5 70.4 70.4 X 68.1 68.1 66.9 69.2 Average, 69.76 69.53 67.46 67.46 Measurements were made with a Spencer movable scale micrometer at a magnification of about 650 diameters. Not all the fertilized eggs remained spherical. For such eggs the longest and shortest diameters are recorded on the table. In computing the averages the mean of these two measurements was taken. It is obvious from the table that a greater amount of water entered the fertilized eggs. This can not be explained by assum- ing an increased permeability to water. It should be noted that the vitelline membranes of the eggs in this experiment did not rupture as a result of being placed in the hypotonic solution. Such a rupture sometimes occurs in solu- tions of this strength. 334 L. V. HEILBRUNN. In hypertonic solutions fertilized Cumingia eggs lose more water than do the unfertilized. The water does not merely leave the eggs more rapidly, more of it passes out and the osmotic equilibrium is different in the two cases. This is shown by an experiment in which fertilized and unfertilized eggs were placed in solutions prepared by adding i part of 2 M MgSO4 to 2 parts of sea-water. Measurements were made as in the previous experiment. The results are given in Table III. TABLE III. DIAMETERS OF FERTILIZED AND UNFERTILIZED EGGS IN 30 c.c. 2M McSOj + 60 c.c. SEA-WATER. Average Diameter of 10 Unfertilized Eggs in Sea-water 6i.~4 n- Fertilized Eggs After 13-23 Min. Fertilized Eggs After 163-172 Min. Unfertilized Eggs After 27-33 Min. Unfertilized Eggs After 143-161 Min. M M M M 57-7 51.9 x 58.3 59-4 60.6 53-7 X 58.9 57-7 6O.O 6O.O 56.5 43-3 59.4 (58.3) 6l.2 (56.0) 57-7 42.7 61.7 58.9 (53-7) 57-1 X 58.3 57-7 60.0 (53.7) 60.6 (56.1) 57-1 (51.9) 56.5 59-4 58.9 56.5 57-1 59-4 59-4 59-5 X 54-2 53-7 60.6 57-7 56.0 53-1 61.2 58.9 55-4 49.0 59-4 59-4 54-8 58.9 X 61.2 59-4 Average, 56.78 5-2-79 60.05 59-55 The above table requires a little explanation. Some of the eggs, usually the unfertilized ones, became slightly flattened at one pole while in the hypertonic solution. These eggs therefore had a slightly smaller volume than their diameter would indicate. In order to show the extent of the flattening a second measure- ment was taken from the flattened pole to the opposite pole of the egg. This second measurement is in every case shown in parentheses. Another point also needs explanation. The ferti- lized eggs measured in the second column had lost so much water in the hypertonic solution that their cytoplasm was coagulated and had in most cases begun to shrink away from the vitelline membrane. This shrinkage was most pronounced in the third and fourth eggs measured and this accounts for the very small size of these eggs. STUDIES IN. ARTIFICIAL PARTHENOGENESIS. 335 The table shows clearly that more water leaves the fertilized than the unfertilized eggs. This can not be due to an increased permeability to water for an increase of this sort could produce no such effect. It must be due to a loss in the rigidity of the plasma membrane. In order to make this relation clear, I shall quote from p. 1 54 of the second paper of this series : ' ' The plasma- membrane of the Arbacia egg is a protein gel. As such it pos- sesses a certain degree of rigidity. Suppose a hypothetical system completely surrounded by an extremely rigid semi- permeable membrane. If such a system were placed in a con- centrated solution no exosmosis could take place, for if the membrane were perfectly rigid, there could be no removal of solvent from the system without the production of a vacuum. But the membrane would be subjected to a considerable pressure which would tend to make it rearrange its particles in such a fashion that the volume enclosed within it might be lessened. Whereas an extremely rigid membrane would resist such forces one with only a certain degree of rigidity would yield (in the case of sufficient pressure) and exosmosis would be possible. Thus osmosis in an enclosed system depends to some extent at least on the rigidity of the confining membrane. These con- clusions apply in some measure to the sea-urchin egg, for the vitelline membrane possesses a slight degree of rigidity." They apply even more directly to the Cumingia egg, for its vitelline membrane, which is also its plasma .membrane, is stifter than that of Arbacia. Any loss in the rigidity of this membrane favors either endosmosis or exosmosis. That is why the fertilized eggs of Cumingia take up more water from hypotonic solutions and lose more water to hypertonic sloutions than do the unfertilized eggs. Osmotic change in unfertilized eggs is fairly rapid and no one can deny that the plasma membrane is permeable to water. If then R. S. Lillie's measurements are correct and water enters fertilized eggs more rapidly than unfertilized eggs in hypotonic solutions and leaves them more rapidly in hypertonic solutions than it seems certain that the egg plasma membrane has not markedly increased its permeability to dissolved substances as a result of fertilization. For such an increase in permeability, 336 L. V. HEILBRUXX. by diminishing the osmotic pressure, would slow osmotic inter- change and if sufficiently great, would prevent it altogether. All these points show clearly that the permeability theory of fertilization and artificial parthenogenesis rests on rather doubtful evidence. In Cumingia certainly, there are no facts which support it. DISCUSSION. Bataillon ('12) in discussing the relations between artificial parthenogenesis in amphibia and sea-urchins, states, "II n'y a pas une parthenogenese experimental des Oursins et une des Amphibiens. Ce sont des materiaux differents chez lesquels le rythme des cineses est suspendu et pent etre retabli. S'il y a une Biologic generale, les conditions de 1'arret ont quelque chose de commun, et les conditions de la mise en branle doivent etre comparable." Presumably this is true. The essential factors underlying stimulation to development are very probably alike for every sort of artificial parthenogenesis. It is certain, how- ever, that the subsidiary features of the process are different in each case. It is necessary therefore to study each egg indi- vidually, to determine exactly its physical make-up, and to attempt to discover what changes are significant in producing an initiation of development. This is what I tried to do in the case of Arbacia. F. R. Lillie ('19) in referring to my explanation of the process of cortical change in Arbacia points out that this explanation "can hardly apply to other cases where the cortical changes present a different morphological form." This is true and I never intended that it should. As a matter of fact, I clearly recognized that even in the one egg there were two distinct types of cortical change which might be produced by spermato- zoa. The explanation which F. R. Lillie cites was only advanced to cover one of them. At the same time I offered a different explanation for the other. F. R. Lillie also objects to my considering cortical change as "a mere epiphenomenon . . . the phenomenon of the primary cortical change is too general to be treated in this fashion and its character in different animal groups is too varied for it to be a mere phenomenon of decrease of surface tension." I must point STUDIES IN ARTIFICIAL PARTHENOGENESIS. 337 out that I have always realized the importance of cortical change. On page 183 of my 1915 paper I stated in a section devoted to the significance of cortical change, that "cortical change, whether it be membrane swelling or elevation, always results in the re- moval of this obstacle (i.e., a stiff membrane]. The vitelline membrane is either rendered soft by swelling or it is lifted away from the egg surface and its place taken by the no doubt less rigid hyaline layer." I showed moreover that "at least two processes which play a part in normal development would be greatly hindered if some kind of cortical change did not occur." My results with Cumingia fully bear out this point of view. Cortical change in Cumingia may take the form of a membrane elevation dependent on a sharp decrease in surface tension. It may more simply be just a membrane swelling. In both cases the result is the same. The egg is freed from an obstacle which impedes development. This in Cumingia as in Arbacia is the restraining influence of a stiff vitelline membrane. In both eggs the" same forces are involved in cortical change. The essential features of the process and the effect on further development are as closely alike as they could possibly be in the two cases. It should be noted that cortical change in Cumingia is not ordinarily followed by segmentation. As in Arbacia cell-division in Cumingia is preceded by a sharp increase in the viscosity of the cytoplasm. This can be demonstrated by tests with the centri- fuge. A number of such tests were made and the relation established beyond a doubt. SUMMARY. 1. The Cumingia egg is surrounded by a stiff vitelline mem- brane which tightly encloses the fluid cytoplasm. 2. A release from the restraint of this membrane is followed by maturation. 3. Such a release from restraint can be accomplished in three ways; by membrane elevation, by membrane swelling, or by the removal or rupture of the membrane. 4. Substances which themselves have low surface tension pro- duce a lowered surface tension of the membrane and this results in its elevation from the egg surface. 338 L. V. HEILBRUNN. 5. Acids, alkalies, and certain salt solutions cause the vitelline membrane to swell. 6. The membrane may be removed from the eggs by shaking, or it may be caused to rupture by immersion in dilute sea-water. 7. All of the above mentioned treatments produce polar-body formation. All of them free the egg from restraint. 8. Maturation in Cumingia is not dependent on an increase in oxidations. 9. Cortical change in Cumingia produces no increase in permea- bility either to dissolved substances' or to water. 10. The essential features of cortical change in Cumingia are the same as those previously shown for Arbacia. BIBLIOGRAPHY. Bataillon, E. '12 La parthenogenese des amphibians et la "fecondation chimique" de Loeb (etude analytique). Ann. des Sci. Nat. Zool., Ser. 9, T. 16, pp. 249-307. Gray, J. '16 The Electrical Conductivity of Echinoderm Eggs, and its Bearing on the Problems of Fertilization and Artificial Parthenogenesis. Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc., Series B, Vol. 207, pp. 481-529. Heilbrunn, L. V. '13 Studies in Artificial Parthenogenesis. I. Membrane Elevation in the Arbacia Egg. BIOL. BULL., Vol. 24, pp. 343-361. '15 Studies in Artificial Parthenogenesis. II. Physical Changes in the Egg of Arbacia. BIOL. BULL., Vol. 29, pp. 149-203. '17 Maturation and Initiation of Development in Cumingia. Proc. Amer. Assn. Anat. Anat., Rec., Vol. n, pp. 362-363. Lillie, F. R. '19 Problems of Fertilization. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. Lillie, R. S. '16 Increase of Permeability to Water Following Normal and Artificial Activa- tion in Sea-Urchin Eggs. Am. Journ. of Physiol., Vol. 40, pp. 249-66. '17 The Conditions Determining the Rate of Entrance of Water into Fertilized and Unfertilized Arbacia Eggs, and the General Relation of Permeability to Activation. Ibid., Vol. 43, pp. 43-57. '18 The Increase of Permeability to Water in Fertilized Sea-Urchin Eggs and the Influence of Cyanide and Anesthetics upon this Change. Ibid., Vol. 45, pp. 406-30. Loeb, J. '13 Artificial Parthenogenesis and Fertilization. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. Lyon, E. P. and Shackell, L. F. '10 On the Increased Permeability of Sea-Urchin Eggs Following Fertilization. Science, N. S., Vol. 32, pp. 249-251. STUDIES IN ARTIFICIAL PARTHENOGENESIS. 339 McClendon, J. G. '10 On the Dynamics of Cell Division. II. Changes in Permeability of Developing Eggs to Electrolytes. Am. Journ. of Physiol., Vol. 27, pp. 240-75- Morgan, T. H. '10 Cytological Studies of Centrifuged Eggs. Journ. Exp. Zool., Vol. 9, pp. 593-655. Runnstrom, J. 'n Untersuchungen iiber die Permeabilitat des Seeigeleies fur Farbstoffe. I. Arkiv for Zoologi, Bd. 7, No. 13, pp. 1-17. Wilson, E. B. '01 Experimental Studies in Cytology. II. Some Phenomena of Fertilization and Cell-division in Etherized Eggs. III. The Effect on Cleavage of Artificial Obliteration of the ist Cleavage Furrow. Arch. f. Entwick- lungsmech., Bd. 13, pp. 353~95- A CONTRIBUTION TO THE LIFE HISTORY OF AMCEBA PROTEUS LEIDY.1 LEON AUGUSTUS HAUSMAN, PH.D., ZOOLOGICAL LABORATORY, CORNELL UNIVERSITY. It is only within the last few years that a connected life history of Amceba proteus has begun to be formulated. This does not seem strange, even in view of its common use in laboratories, since the difficulties attendant upon the study of the life cycle of such an elusive form, are numerous. The isolated observations upon the various activities of Amceba, the accounts of suppositions appendages, and the descriptions of supposedly new species or varieties, are numerous. The fact that so much observational material upon so many apparently nearly related forms has accumulated, and that so many diverse accounts of the habits of one and the same form have been given, has led some investigators to suspect that perhaps much of this material might be combined to throw light upon the life history of possibly some one or two valid species in whose life cycles some of the forms heretofore described might prove to be merely developmental stages. This suspicion has been strength- ened recently by the fact that some of the Rhizopoda closely allied to the Amceba proteus, whose life cycles are now known, show both widely varying forms and habits during different stages in their growth and reproduction. Investigations of the life history of Amceba proteus which have hitherto been made make it possible to generalize, to some extent, upon the course of some of the changes which take place during its development. There seem to be several modes of reproduction in this form, and several intermediate stages be- tween young and adult before the life cycle is completed. In this communication an attempt is made; first, to review briefly what we know of the life history of Amoeba proteus; second, to 1 Grateful acknowledgment is here made of the aid of Professor H. D. Reed, of the department of zoology, Cornell University. 340 LIFE HISTORY OF AMCEBA PROTEUS LEIDY. 34! describe an apparently hitherto unknown method of reproduc- tion; and third to record some observations upon the develop- ment and growth of the forms produced by this new method. The first observer to suggest that another method of reproduc- tion other than simple fission might obtain among the Amceba was Varter ('56) who recorded the presence of numerous fine granules which he observed filling the bodies of some Amceba radiosa. These granules he looked upon as fragments of the nucleus, and since, when they escaped from the body, they moved with a curious jerky motion, he inferred that they must be provided with flagella, and termed them spermatozoids. The observations extended no farther. Again in 1863 he observed the granulation of the nucleus in Amceba princeps, noting that the original nucleus divided several times and gave rise to many smaller nuclei, which he termed, though apparently with meager foundation, reproductive cells. Wallich ('63) noted that when Amceba villosa "died," the nucleus, which had divided several times, escaped from the body surrounded by bits of protoplasm, of globular form. He observes that the fate of these globules is unknown to him, but believing that they might function in some reproductive process, termed them scaroblasts. Referring to the many different species of Amceba which appeared in his cultures, he writes (with reference particularly to Amceba princeps, diffluens, and radiosa} : " It will, I think, eventually be found that these are mere transitory phases of one and the same species." Later, he observes with greater conviction: "... though not prepared to affirm that the whole of the varieties of Amcebce are reducible to a single, primary, specific type, I candidly confess that the balance of the evidence appears to me to point to such a conclusion, and to indicate that the divergence in the form and outward characters may be wholly dependent on the local and even temporary conditions of the medium in which the young animal happens to make its appearance in the world." It is to Scheel ('99) that we owe our first complete knowledge of a type of reproduction different from the familiar method of binary fission. In 1899 he described a process of reproduction in Amceba proteus which he termed schizogony, after a somewhat 342 LEON AUGUSTUS HAUSMAN. similar mode of multiplication occurring among the Sporozoa. As it occurs in Amoeba protects its course is as follows: The individual encysts; the nucleus divides into several smaller nuclei; these migrate to the periphery of the cell; the protoplasm of the cell divides into as many equal portions as there are nuclei; the cyst wall ruptures; and the nucleus-containing bits of protoplasm emerge, each a complete Amoeba and an epitome of its parent. In analogy with the spores of the Sporozoa these were given the name, pseudopodiospores. More recently Calkins ('05) has noted a method of reproduction in which the nucleus produced by repeated division, gametic nuclei, whose fusion resulted in other nuclei which became the nuclei of the smaller individuals after the manner of the pseudopodiospores just men- tioned. The young Amcebce soon assumed the form hitherto called Amoeba radiosa; passed through this stage and became the common Amoeba proteus. Another apparent mode of reproduction was observed by Metcalf ('10). In this method the observer is of the opinion that the sequence of events is: Mature individuals produce globular masses of protoplasm, which are termed gemmules. These develop flagella and assume a cercomonadoid form; later they fuse by two and two, lose the flagella, and develop into adult Amceboz. It is stated that possibly the life cycle of Amoeba proteus may require a year or more for its completion, and may exhibit during its course three, or even more, modes of repro- duction. Schepotieff ('08, '09) states: "All of these examples suggest that in the case of the Amoeba [proteus] the developmental cycle may be completed in very different ways." It has been shown, as we have said, that Amoeba radiosa, formerly regarded as a distinct species, is merely a stage in the development of Amoeba proteus. It may be that this also is true of such recognized species of Amoeba as villosa, princeps, diffluens, etc. It is suggested elsewhere in this paper that the species known as Amoeba guttula is but a developmental stage in the cycle of proteus. Vahlkampf ('05) has shown that Amoeba Umax, at least, is to be regarded as a distinct species. LIFE HISTORY OF AMCEBA PROTEUS LEIDY. 343 PREPARATION OF CULTURES. The material from which the Amcebce herein described were reared was obtained from a pool in a cattail marsh, in about two feet of water among decaying lily pads and ceratophyHum. The thin glutinous deposit upon the bottom and especially on the lily pads which had fallen to the bottom was found to be rich in Amoeba proteus. In the laboratory the material was dis- tributed into several battery jar aquaria after having been filtered through cheese cloth to remove the larger creatures. An immediate examination of the material showed that beside the active proteus, radiosa, gultula, and Umax, there were many encysted protozoa. One encysted form which appeared in large numbers, and which confusingly resembles some of the smaller encysted Amcebce was Vorticella microstoma (Fig. 4). Under ordinary magnification its distinguishing feature, the crescentric nucleus, is not visible. With the 1.8 mm. objective it becomes faintly discernible, but it is best seen, and the cyst is indubitably identified apparently only when stained. Methyl green and iodine gave the best results.1 After some weeks had elapsed proteus and radiosa appeared in large numbers. The material was now transferred to a dozen small petri dishes and kept in a constant temperature of about 75 degrees Fahr. After a space of a fortnight there was begun the transfer of inoculation of Amoeba proteus to 4 cm. stender dishes, furnished with straw infusion or oak leaf infusion, and free from all protozoan forms. The infusions were prepared by boiling the straw or leaves for several hours, and decanting off the dark brown liquor, to be diluted to the optimum strength. A slimy scum formed upon the surface of the infusions after a few days time, which when stirred up and caused to sink to the bottom furnished a nutritive substance upon which the Amcebce throve. From the stender dishes individual Amcebce were removed from time to time and kept in shallow cells sunk in slides ot unusual thickness. The slides employed were furnished with a device 1 Methyl green stain: Saturated alcoholic solution methyl green, 3 parts; 2 per cent, aqueous solution acetic acid, i part; water 3 parts. Iodine stain: saturated alcoholic solution iodine, i part; water, 2 parts. For various protozoan stains, see Hausman, "Fresh Water and Marine Gymnostominan Infusoria" (in press). 344 LEON AUGUSTUS HAUSMAN. for supplying water to take the place of that carried off by evaporation (Fig. 3). The method of removing individual Amoeba from stender dishes was as follows, and can be used with success for the isola- tion of any of the larger protozoan forms : A drop of water con- taining the Amozbtz was placed upon an ordinary slide, and, uncovered, searched with the 16 mm. objective and 8 X eye- piece. When Amoeba were located they were removed by means of what is termed an isolation pipette (Fig. 2). A long rubber tube attached at one end to a glass tube drawn out to a very fine tip, and at the other to a small compression bulb, enables one to select and withdraw very minute objects with considerable precision. Both the stender dishes and growing slides were kept at a temperature of 80 degrees Fahr. in a large aquarium jar heated by a carbon filament lamp placed in the bottom, the current being controlled by a rheostat. By means of this simple device unvarying temperatures could be maintained for any desired length of time. The advantage of such a culture oven is that light is admitted freely on all sides (Fig. i). FORMATION OF APSEUDOPODIOSPORES. One of the stender dishes which had been inoculated with adult Amoeba protens proved very productive, the individuals increasing rapidly, apparently by means of binary fission, since many were observed in process of division. The bodies of the largest individuals became filled with minute bodies, which upon staining seemed to be nuclei. The individuals bearing these were extremely sluggish. The pseudopodia were short ; exhibited very little movement, changed their shape but slowly, and upon the functional posterior of the body absent altogether (Fig. 5). After an interval of four days had elapsed a reexamination of some of the sediment from the same stender dish was made and it was found that the numbers of the large proteus had appre- ciably decreased and that their places were taken by a multitude of very small amoeboid forms, of an average diameter of 4 or 5 microns. The majority of these exhibited feeble movements of an amoeboid kind. Some were globular, some possessed an irregular body outline, though definite pseudopodia were lacking LIFE HISTORY OF AMCEBA PROTEUS LEIDY. 345 (Fig. 10). These, I was later led to believe, originated from the bodies of the larger multinucleated proteus. Several of these latter individuals while under observation were observed, to give rise to smaller individuals of the form already described. The process was as follows: the animal, which had been moving slowly and apparently without much vigor, gradually came to rest with the pseudopodia upon the then functional anterior portion of the body lobate with slightly pointed tips (Fig. 6). The posterior portion became semi-globular and towards this the greater number of the minute suppositive nuclei within the body plasm migrated. After an interval of about seven to ten minutes the ectoplasm surrounding this globular posterior ex- tremity appeared to disintegrate and from the interior there floated forth several hyaline globules about 4 or 5 microns in diameter. These were followed, after a few minutes by several others, and then a constant outflow began that continued until upwards of thirty of the hyaline spheroids had been extruded (Fig. 7). These were apparently identical with the forms which had made their appearance in the culture. Since these are judged not to be fundamentally different from the pseudopodiospores, of Scheel, but since they exhibited no lobose pseudopodia, they are here called for convenience, apseudopodios pores. Some of these, immediately after extrusion began to move slowly, bulging the very thin ectoplasm at several points, yet without forming any definite pseudopodia. Others, synchronous with their emergence, disintegrated. Still others floated away without exhibiting any signs of motion (Fig. 8). It may be that these were gametic forms, and fused before further development. Towards the time of the completion of apseudopodiospore ejection, the parent Amcebce usually gave signs of renewed ac- tivity, elongated perceptibly, and then began apparently to make efforts to move away (Fig. 9). However, after an interval of from twenty minutes to half an hour, they disintegrated. . Some disintegrated at once, leaving the apseudopodiospores behind, but usually they were extruded from the globular posterior portion of the animal, the anterior part retaining its integrity. It appeared as though not all of the minute nuclei were used up at the time of the production of one "litter" of apseudopodio- 346 LEON AUGUSTUS HAUSMAN. spores, and that some of them (those in the anterior portion) were lost. M. Popoff ('u) has recorded a type of reproduction in Amoeba minuta, a marine species, similar to the one described above, with the difference, however, that the resulting spores were gametes, which later fused. Schmidt ('13) likewise, described this same sort of reproductive activity as occurring in another marine species, Amasba aquitalis. The apseudopodiospores however, at least the majority whose development was watched, were not gametes. The smallest of the young Amcebce (as we shall call them) those which have just been separated from the parent body are about 3 to 5 microns in diameter and are extremely sluggish. The body is sub-globular and changes its outline but little during the very slow movement. No pseudopodia are developed. The protoplasm is clear, and contains a few small, angular, trans- parent granules. No contractile vacuole could be seen, nor where the creatures observed to feed (Fig. 10). With growth comes an increase in activity and a progressively greater irregularity of the body outline (Fig. n), until at length true lobate pseudopodia make their appearance (Fig. 12). The number of granules within the body increases, food is taken by engulfing, and the protoplasm assumes a grayish hue. This color may be due both to the number of particles within the endoplasm and to the augmentation of its volume. At the time of the appearance of the true pseudopodia the body is unsymmetrical, but as growth proceeds are more or less radiate arrangement of the pseudopodia takes place, at first not well defined, but becoming more and more pronounced with the increase in size (Fig. 13). The pseudopodia now become more extended, and tend to develop more acuminate tips. With increasing length and sharp- ness the pseudopods seem to become more rigid, and spine like, and the granules migrate from them into the more globular central mass of the body, leaving them clear (Fig. 14). During the time when the young Amcebce are passing from the apseudopodia stage to the radiosa stage they confusingly resemble, if indeed they are not exactly similar to the species LIFE HISTORY OF AMCEBA PROTEUS LEIDY. 347 known as Amoeba guttula (Fig. n), and it is suggested that possibly this creature hitherto accorded specific rank, may be merely a developmental stage in the cycle of proteus. That Amoeba radiosa was named from the radiosa stage of Amoeba proteus has been indicated. The radiosa stage which we have observed in this developmental series may be similar. A new type of modification now takes place, as has been said, when the pseudopodia become longer and more spinous. During this stage, in respect to size, configuration, and characteristic spineous, immobile, hyaline, ray-like pseudopodia, the creatures are apparently indistinguishable from the species known as Dactylosph&rium radiosum. Hence we shall term this stage the Dactylosphcerium stage (Fig. 15). The genus Dactylosphcerium was established by Hertwig, and Lesser ('74) to receive the organism which they described as Dactylosphcerium vitreum. The species now known as Dactylo- sphcerium radiosum, however, was not referred originally to that genus, but to the genus Amceba, as Amoeba radiosa, by Ehrenberg ('30). It was transferred to the genus Dactylosphcerium by Biitschli ('80), who however erroneously called it Dactylosphceria. Cash ('05) says of it: "The body consists of granular protoplasm and when all the pseudopodia are withdrawn it may become spherical or bluntly lobed ; or it may assume an active amoeboid phase, when it is hardly, if at all,1 to be distinguished from the smaller forms of Amceba proteus." It occurs in marshes and pools, "less common than Amoeba proteus, with which it is usually associated." 2 It was observed that not all of the young Amcebce acquired this Dactylosphcerium-\ike form. Some became small radiosa, passed on to large radiosa, and thence on to the proteus form. It seemed to be the usual thing, however, for the majority to assume the form of Dactylosphcerium, and the suggestion is made that perhaps Dactylosphcerium radiosum, like other former species, may be forced to relinquish its specific distinction. From the bodies of those individuals which had assumed the Dactylosphcerium form, lobate pseudopodia were occasionally pro- truded, and at length the creature became almost globular, and - The italics are the author's. 348 LEON AUGUSTUS HAUSMAN. then proceeded to take on a form similar to the adult proteus. Such individuals were isolated, and soon grew in size to adult form, and were, apparently, Amceba proteus. Fission was ob- served to take place among some of these, but no further repro- ductive activities were noted. The entire sequence of events which have been enumerated took place within three months, the cultures being kept in the glass oven already described, at an average temperature of 80 degrees Fahr. Some of the individuals of nearly adult proportions developed long, whip-like, and flexible pseudopodia, often more like long threads (Fig. 16). These were clear, even the stouter ones being devoid of granules. Gruber ('n) reported a similar type of pseudopodium (there was but one in this case) which was sent out from the body and moved about as if an organ of exploration. Some of the most bizarre forms occur during the transition period between the smallest form and the Dactylosphcerium stage. Fig. 14 represents forms, all but one of which were taken from the same slide. In Fig. 1 8 an attempt has been made to group the various forms observed in a tentative cycle of development. The nine arbitrary stages appear to occur in the following order: 1. Adult stage. 2. Division of the nucleus, and migration of the nuclei to the posterior extremity of the body. 3. Escape of apseudopodiospores. 4. Amceba guttula stage. 5. Small Amceba proteus stage. 6. Amceba radiosa stage. 7. Dactylosphcerium radiosum stage. 8. Resumption of amoeboid form. 9. Growth to adult size. BIBLIOGRAPHY. Biitschli, O. '80 Protozoa, in Bronn, H. G. Klassen und Ordnungen des Thier Reichs, parts 8 and 9. Calkins, G. N. '05 Evidences of a Sexual Cycle in the Life History of Amoeba proteus. Archiv fur Protistenkunde, Vol. 5, p. i. '07 Fertilization of Amtzba proteus. BIOL. BULL., Woods Hole, Mass., Vol. 13. P- 219. LIFE HISTORY OF AMOEBA PROTEUS LEIDY. 349 Carter, H. J. '56 Notes on the Freshwater Infusoria of the Island of Bombay. Ann. & Mag. of Nat. Hist., Vol. 18, p. 115. '63 On Amoeba princeps and Its Reproductive Cells. Ann. & Mag. of Nat Hist., Vol. 12, p. 30. Cash, J. '05 British Rhizopoda and Hdiozoa. London. Ehrenberg, H. '30 Beitrage zur Kenntniss der Organization der Infusorien, etc. Abhl. der Konigl. Akad. der Wiss. Berlin, p. 30. Griiber. 'n Ueber Einartige Korperformen von Amoeba proteus. Archiv. fur Protisten- kunde, Vol. 23, p. 253. Hertwig & Lesser. '74 Ueber Rhizopoden Denselben Nahstehende Organismen. Archiv. fur Mikr. Anat., Vol. 10, suppl., p. 35. Metcalf, M. '10 Studies upon Amoeba. Jour. Ex. Zool., Vol. 9, p. 311. Penard. '02 Faune Rhizopodique du Bassin du Leman, Geneva. Popoff, M. 'n Ueber der Entwicklungscyclus von Amoeba minuta. Archiv fur Protisten- kunde, Vol. 22, p. 179. Schaudinn, F. '03 Untersuchungen ueber die Fortpflanzen einiger Rhizopoden. Arbeit aus dem Kaiserl. Gesundheitsamte, Vol. 19, p. 547. Scheel, C. '99 Beitrage zur Fortpflanzung der Amceben. Festschrift zur Siebensigsten Geburtstag von Carl von Kupffer, Jena, p. 569. Schepotieff, A. 'og-'io Amcebenstudien. Zool. Jahrbucher, Abt. fur Anat., Vol. 29, p. 485. Schmidt, H. '13 Faunistische und Entwicklungsgeschichtliche^Studien an Sarcodinen der Umgegend von Bonn. Archiv fur Protistenkunde, Vol. 29, p. 203. Wallich, C. G. '63 (a) On an Undescribed Indigenous Form of Amoeba. Ann. & Mag. of Nat. Hist., Vol. n, p. 287. (b) Further Observations on A. villosa and other Indigenous Rhizopods. Ann. & Mag. of Nat. Hist., Vol. n, p. 434. Vahlkampf, E. '05 Beitrage zur biologic und Entwicklungsgeschichte von Amoeba Umax. Archiv fur Protistenkunde, Vol. 5, p. 167. Vejdovsky, F. '80 Ueber die Rhizopoden der Brunnewasser Prags. Sitsber. der bohm. Gesell. der Wiss. Prag., p. 136. 35° LEON AUGUSTUS HAUSMAN. PLATE I. FIG. i. Aquarium jar culture oven, a, thermometer; b, cardboard cover; c, aquarium jar; d, cultures; e, iron tripod; /, lamp; g, copper wire rack for lamp. FIG. 2. Isolation pipette. FIG. 3. Growing slide, a, sponge, for absorbing excess water; b, cover glass; c, glass tube carrying thread, as siphon; d, lower half of glass vial, cemented to the slide with balsam, as reservoir. FIG. 4. Vorticella microstoma, encycted. FIG. 5. Unusually large Amoeba proteus, with body filled with minute nuclei. FIG. 6. Proteus, resting before extrusion of the apseudopodiospores. FIG. 7. Extrusion of the apseudopodiospores. FIG. 8. Immobile, globular apseudopodiospores. FIG. 9. Proteus after extrusion of the apseudopodiospores. FIG. 10. Active apseudopodiospores. FIG. ii. Apseudopodiospores during the stage when they resemble Amceba gultula. FIG. 12. Small proteus stage. FIG. 13. Amoeba radiosa stage. FIG. 14. Apparent transitional forms between the radiosa and the Dactylo- spha:rium stages. BIOLOGICAL BULLETIN, VOL. XXXVlll. PLATE I. L. A. HAU8MAN. 352 LEON AUGUSTUS HAUSMAN. PLATE II. FIG. 15. Dactylosphcerium radiosum stage. FIG. 16. Amoeba proleus in the adult stage, exhibiting long attenuated pseudo- podia. FIG. 17. Adult normal Amoeba proleus (stained). FIG. 18. Tentative Cycle of Development, i. Adult stage. 2. Division of nucleus; migration of nuclei to posterior. 3. Extrusion of the apseudopodiospores. 4. Amceba guttula stage. 5. Small A mxba proteus stage. 6. Amoeba radiosa stage. 7. Dactylosph&rium radiosum stage. 8. Resumption of amoeboid form; retraction of pseudopodia. 9. Growth to adult. BIOLOGICAL BULLETIN, VOL. XXXVIII. PLATE 17 16 18 L. A. HAUSMAN. Vol. XXXVIII. June, 1920. No. 6 BIOLOGICAL BULLETIN THE AXIAL GRADIENTS IN HYDROZOA. III. EX- PERIMENTS ON THE GRADIENT OF TUBULARIA. LIBBIE H. HYMAN, HULL ZOOLOGICAL LABORATORY, UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO. A. INTRODUCTION. In a recent paper Banus ('18) states that there is no difference between the time of regeneration of oral hydranths on apical and basal pieces of the stem of Tnbularia. The data presented by Banus apparently support this conclusion. Such data are, how- ever, of no significance unless complete information is given as to the manner in which they are obtained. Perusal of Banus's paper reveals the fact that absolutely no information is imparted concerning the conditions under which the experiments were performed or the manner of handling the material. Those who have worked on the physiology of the lower forms are well aware that experimental results can be readily controlled and modified by conditions. It is, therefore, impossible for the impartial mind to accept the validity of Banus's conclusions, until further information concerning his experiments shall be forthcoming. Grave doubt is cast upon the correctness of Banus's statements by the fact, completely ignored by him, that experiments of this kind had already been performed several times, with results contrary to his. In addition to these omissions, Banus has made a number of exaggerated and misleading statements. Banus begins by saying that Child "assumes the existence of metabolic gradients in a great number of species of animals and plants and on this assumption he builds a theory of individual- ity." In view of the great mass of data which has been presented concerning metabolic gradients and the extent to which these 353 354 LIB3IE H. HYMAN. data have been checked by several different methods it seems hardly scientific to dismiss the matter under the word "assumes." Gradients are not assumptions; they are facts. It is legitimate, of course, for any one to question and criticize the interpretation of these facts, and desirable that other possible explanations of them should be suggested ; but to ignore such facts by designating them as assumptions is not the way to arrive at scientific truth. The nature of the axial gradients has been so thoroughly and frequently discussed in numerous papers from this laboratory that presentation of the subject here seems to me superfluous. Banus next remarks that the hydroid Tubularia is extensively used to support Child's conceptions. It is scarcely necessary to point out to the zoological world the exaggeration conveyed by this statement, as it is well known that other ccelenterates have been used as extensively and other lower forms much more extensively in accumulating the experimental evidence upon which those conceptions rest. We are next informed that Child "has made no measurements of the rate of metabolism of different regions of the stem of Tubularia." We would be pleased to carry out such experiments if Banus would kindly suggest a suitable method. The matter would be relatively simple were it not for the fact that the relative proportions of perisarc and cosnosarc vary in different regions of the stem of Tubularia. It is therefore difficult or impossible to determine the amount of living material in portions of the stem and impossible to establish any basis for comparison of the metabolism of different regions. Naked hydroids, such as Corymorpha, would be required for experiments of this kind. The metabolic rate of different regions of the first zooid of Planaria dorotocephala has been determined and has been found to accord with the metabolic gradient conception. Banus then proceeds to discuss the regional differences in rate of regeneration of Tubularia. He says that Child "as- sumes" the existence of such differences. In his summary he states that "the rate of regeneration of the oral hydranth of an apical piece is on the average identical with the rate of re- generation of the oral hydranth of the basal piece"; and further that "there is no evidence of the existence of level or regional THE AXIAL GRADIENTS IN HYDROZOA. 355 differences in the stem of Tubularia." Such statements as these display an unpardonable ignorance of the literature dealing with the regeneration of Tubularia, This form has been in- vestigated by a number of well-known zoologists, and twenty years ago the regional differences which Banus denies were demonstrated to exist. The first experiments on the regeneration of Tubularia were those of Loeb ('91 and '92), performed at Naples. In the first of these publications (on p. 15) he states that he took very long stems, cut off the roots and polyps, and then divided the remain- ing portion in half. No difference wras observed in the rate of regeneration of the oral hydranths on the apical and basal pieces. FIG. i. Diagram of Tubularia to illustrate method of cutting apical and basa pieces of the stem of equal length; employed in experiments recorded in Tables III., IV., V., VII., IX., and X. a, apical; b, basal. The following year he repeated the experiment and this time he observed that in one experiment (he does not state how many experiments were performed) the oral hydranths emerged about twenty-four hours earlier on the apical than on the basal pieces. Owing to the meager details which are furnished regarding these experiments, it is impossible to determine why the same investi- gator working on the same material should at one time obtain 356 LIBBIE H. HYMAN. one result and on another occasion the contrary result. Loeb has chosen to disregard the one contrary experiment since later ("Organism as a Whole," p. 171), he again asserts, in spite of all the evidence at that time available to the contrary, that apical and basal pieces of the stem of Tubularia regenerate hydranths simultaneously. This statement has never been confirmed, ex- cept by Banus; every other zoologist who has worked upon the matter has found the contrary to be true. In 1899, Driesch working also at Naples expressed himself as fully convinced that Loeb was mistaken. He found many evidences of regional differences in Tubularia. He observed that the length of the primordium is greater the nearer the piece is to the original distal end of the stem; that in very small pieces, the more apical pieces produce larger hydranths and tend to give rise to distal structures only, while the basal pieces produce smaller hydranths and proximal structures; and that when long pieces of the stem are cut in half, the apical halves give rise to oral hydranths earlier than the basal halves. Driesch, therefore, as he em- phatically stated in this paper, disagreed with Loeb on this point. In Table X, p. 131, Driesch gives a record of thirty pieces in which the apical halves regenerated oral hydranths one to twenty-three hours earlier than the basal halves in twenty- five cases and simultaneously with them in but five cases. Table XI, p. 132, presents similar data. These statements of Driesch were verified by Morgan ('01, '05, 'o6a, '08), and by Morgan and Stevens ('04). Thus Morgan says ('05, p. 496),— "the rate of both oral and aboral development is determined by the level at which the end lies." Again in 1906, p. 497, he states: "It has been shown in Tubularia that the time required for the formation of a new hydranth depends on the distance of the cut surface from the old hydranth. The nearer the cut surface to the oral end the quicker the regeneration. The same law also holds for the development of the aboral hydranth from the aboral end of a piece." These statements were reiterated in 1908, p. 157 — physiologically polarity is "shown in the more rapid regeneration of the cut surfaces the nearer they are to the distal end." Child ('07) again performed the experiment in question and THE AXIAL GRADIENTS IN HYDROZOA. 357 agreed with the results obtained by Driesch, Morgan, and Stevens. These experiments of Child's are the only ones considered by Banus and the false impression is thereby conveyed that no one but Child had ever experimented upon the matter. Banus has criticized these experiments of Child's on the grounds that they are not extensive and that the differences recorded are in some cases so slight that they may be due to experimental error. It A B FIG. 2. Diagram to show method of cutting unequal pieces for the experiments given in Table VI. 2A, apical pieces half as long as basal pieces; 2B, apical pieces twice as long as basal pieces. is true that no very extensive series of experiments were carried out because Child felt that the point had already been settled by the work of Driesch and Morgan and that any further experi- mentation was superfluous. Such observations as he made were therefore incidental to other matters. It is also true, as Banus says, that the differences obtained were not very large, but since they always vary in the same direction, the results cannot be due, as claimed by Banus, to experimental variation. In Table 358 LIBBIE H. HYMAN. I., p. 2, of Child's paper we find that of 24 pairs of pieces, the distal oral hydranths emerged first in 14 cases, at the same time as the proximal oral hydranths in 5 cases, and later than the latter in 5 cases. A result which is due to experimental error should vary equally in both directions. It should also be stated that part of the pieces given in this table were regenerating in modified sea-water and not under normal conditions. In regard to Tables II. and III., p. 5, Banus has misrepresented the facts. In these tables the differences between the times of emergence of distal and proximal oral hydranths are less than in the pre- ceding table, although they still vary in the same direction; but it is distinctly stated by Child in the text that the difference is decreased owing to the smaller size of the pieces, and he further shows that with still greater reduction in the length of the pieces, the proximal oral hydranths will emerge first. In short, these experiments were directed towards demonstrating the effect of reduced length on the time of emergence of the oral hydranths, a fact which Banus in quoting them omits to mention. After Banus's paper appeared the experiment was repeated at Woods Hole in the summer of 1918 by Dr. W. C. Allee. Dr. Alice was entirely unable to agree with Banus's statements, but found, on the contrary, that the oral hydranths arise earlier on apical than on basal pieces of the stem of Tubularia. He com- municated this result to Professor Child and other members of this laboratory and also showed his experiments to a number of people at Woods Hole. In the summer of 1919, Dr. Allee assigned the experiment to his class in Invertebrate Zoology at Woods Hole. Twenty-seven sets of experiments were performed by the students, each set consisting of from two to eight pairs of pieces. Twenty-four hours after cutting, the apical halves (as indicated by the red color of the regenerating ends) were in advance in twenty-five of the sets, the basal halves in advance in one set, and in the other, there was no difference. Of the 112 pairs of pieces cut, 95 of the apical pieces survived, and 71 of the basal pieces. After forty-eight hours, 52 or 55 per cent, of the apical pieces had produced hydranths, while this had occurred in only 15, or 20 per cent, of the basal pieces. It should be stated that the material was not in first-class condition at the THE AXIAL GRADIENTS IN HYDROZOA. 359 time, and, as I shall show, the difference between the time of regeneration of apical and basal pieces is reduced under such circumstances. Professor Child and I are greatly indebted to Dr. Allee for his interest in the matter, and for his kindness in putting through the experiments. In view, therefore, of the overwhelming preponderance of the evidence already at hand in support of the existence of the FIGS. 3 AND 4. Diagrams to show method of cutting pieces for experiments given in Table VIII. Figure 3, method for all experiments in Table VIII., except number 36; figure 4, method used for experiment 36. regional differences along the axis of Tubularia which are denied by Banus, further experimentation seems superfluous. Under the circumstances, however, a repetition of the experiment has been deemed necessary by various members of this laboratory as an answer to Banus's paper. I therefore undertook to repeat his work and for this purpose made trips to Woods Hole in June, and in December, 1919. The results were identical at both 360 LIBBIE H. HYMAN. seasons of the year and were completely at variance with Banus's statements. I was unable to verify any of his results. In numerous experiments conducted for this purpose, the apical halves of Tubularia stems regenerated markedly faster than the basal halves of the same stems. I further believe that I dis- covered the cause of Banus's peculiar results. My results are presented in detail in the present paper. Not only are the researches just enumerated opposed to Banus's statements but a large number of other facts concerning the regeneration of Tubidaria clearly point to the existence of axial differences in metabolic rate in this form. Thus all of the facts collected by Driesch, Morgan, and Child concerning the phenomena of "polarity" in Tubularia are entirely irreconcilable with the view point of Banus and Loeb. If there is no axial difference along the stem of Tubularia, why should the apical end of a piece produce a hydranth and the basal end a stolon, or if a hydranth, only later than the apical end? Why do heteromorphic hydranths arise simultaneously on the two ends of very short pieces while on long pieces the aboral hydranth is delayed? This question has received no adequate answer except that based on the axial gradient conception; in short pieces, there is practically no gradient, and hence each end of the piece begins to produce a hydranth at the same or nearly the same time; while, in long pieces, the apical end by virtue of its higher meta- bolic rate gets the start in hydranth formation and hence gains control of the stem for a certain distance, thus inhibiting the formation of the aboral hydranth. Why, as shown inde- pendently by Driesch, Morgan, and Child, in an axial series of very short pieces, do the apical pieces produce larger distal structures with much reduced or absent proximal structures while the more basal pieces give rise to smaller distal structures and larger proximal parts? Longer pieces from the distal region thus resemble shorter pieces from the proximal region in the structures which they produce, owing to the pronounced tendency of the distal pieces to use up their substance in the formation of distal structures only. W'hy is the primordium of the oral hydranth larger in apical than in basal pieces and the emerged hydranth likewise larger? These and numerous similar facts THE AXIAL GRADIENTS IN HYDROZOA. have been repeatedly ascertained by several investigators and are totally inexplicable on the point of view maintained by Loeb and Banus that there are no regional differences in the stem of Tubularia. There is one further statement made by Banus to which we must take exception. This is the assertion on p. 266 and again on p. 273 that regional differences in rate of regeneration con- stitute the "actual basis" for the axial gradient conception as FIG. 5. Method of cutting pieces usually employed by Banus and used for the experiments recorded in Table XI. applied to Tubularia. They are part of the basis, but not the entire basis, as Banus implies. Axial differences in metabolic rate along the stems of Tubularia had been demonstrated at the time when Banus published his paper by two other methods— the differences in susceptibility to potassium cyanide and other substances, and the differences in electric potential. The results yielded by both of these methods are again considered in this paper. In view of the fact that Banus's results are in total disagree- ment with a considerable number of researches, a critical examina- 362 LIBBIE H. HYMAX. tion of his method of procedure is necessitated. But, as has already been pointed out, this is impossible because no description is given by Banus concerning his material, his methods, or his experimental conditions. We need to know the season of the year, the temperature, the vegetative condition of the material, the length of time material was kept in the laboratory before being used, the presence or absence of lateral buds on the stems employed (since buds mark the limits of the individual), and particularly the level from which the pieces were taken, with reference to the original hydranth. We have repeatedly pointed out that metabolic gradients are not fixed and static things, but markedly dynamic and labile, and particularly in the lower forms, they may result from external conditions and may be readily modified and altered by conditions. The failure of Banus to describe or consider the various factors mentioned, any one of which might alter the experimental result, is evidence that he really does not understand the metabolic gradient conception and has not interested himself in understanding it. This is further shown by certain remarks made in his paper such as for example the naive statement on p. 268 that the pieces were "long enough to show a marked difference according to Child's opinion," whereas in fact "according to Child's opinion," and as even a hasty perusal of the work put out from this laboratory would show, the axial differences are most clearly marked in most respects in relatively short pieces. In order, therefore, to obtain any information concerning the experiments of Banus, it has been necessary for me to com- municate with him. Mr. Banus replied to the first letter which I wrote to him, but did not reply to two others requesting further details. It is therefore not possible for me to furnish all of the details necessary for a correct evaluation of these experiments but the information I was able to obtain is sufficiently astonishing. Banus states that his experiments were performed in New York City in November and in Woods Hole in December. Since most of the other researches on Tubularia were carried out in the summer season, it was at first thought that seasonal differ- ences in the vegetative condition of Tubularia might account for Banus's results. I found, however, that Tubularia is in prac- THE AXIAL GRADIENTS IN HYDROZOA. 363 tically the same condition at Woods Hole in June and in December and yields identical results at these two seasons of the year. Banus further states that the "temperature during re- generation was in New York 22° C., and in Woods Hole it was about 15° C." Upon examination of Banus's tables, one finds that the time interval between section and emergence of hy- dranths is much too great for these temperatures. At a tem- perature of 22° C., apical pieces of Tubularia produce hydranths as early as 36 hours after section and the great majority of such pieces will have completed regeneration in 48-60 hours, yet in no case do any of the pieces recorded in Banus's tables regenerate in less than 53 hours and the majority of them require more than 60 hours. Even at a temperature of 15° C., the times given by Banus are surprisingly long, since I found that at 12° C., the majority of the apical pieces will regenerate within 70-80 hours (see Table IV.). It is therefore evident that in Banus's experi- ments some factor is acting to delay the time of regeneration of the apical pieces, whose regeneration precedes by an average of i o to 12 hours, according to my findings, the regeneration of the basal pieces. The cause of the delayed regeneration in Banus's experiments lies in all probability in his method of cutting the apical pieces. In reply to my inquiry concerning the level of the stem from which he cut the pieces, Banus made the following statement: "the most distal cut was usually made as near as possible to the hydranth without including any part of it." Such a method of procedure explains the aberrant results obtained by Banus. It has long been known that the short stalk below the hydranth of Tubularia is incapable of regeneration. When the apical pieces are cut in the manner described by Banus, this stalk forms the distal end of such pieces. It dies and disintegrates, thereby markedly delaying the regeneration and time of emergence of oral hydranths on these pieces. I shall present evidence in this paper (see Table XI.) that when the apical pieces are cut in such a way that their distal ends are just below the base of the original hydranth, the time of emergence of the oral hydranth is greatly delayed and falls behind that of the basal pieces. Hence when such a procedure is followed, all 364 LIBBIE H. HYMAN. sorts of irregular results are obtained and it is not surprising that under such circumstances, the experimenter would be lead to question the existence of axial differences along the stem of Tubularia. When the correct method of cutting the apical pieces is observed, namely, when the hydranth, its stalk, and the first millimeter or two of the stem are discarded, then there is not the slightest question that the apical halves of stems prepared in this manner give rise to oral hydranths very much in advance of the basal halves. The experiments presented in this paper were performed at Woods Hole in June and in December, 1919. I am greatly indebted to Professor F. R. Lillie for a research room at the Marine Biological Laboratory on both of these occasions and for a grant from the departmental funds covering the expenses of the December trip. I am also indebted to Professor C. M. Child for advice and suggestions throughout the course of the work. B. SUSCEPTIBILITY GRADIENTS IN TUBULARIA. The death gradients in lethal concentrations of various sub- stances have already been described by Child ('19^) for a number of hydroids including Tubularia. I have repeated and confirmed these observations for a number of forms. In Tubularia, the disintegration (i/ioo to 1/400 mol.KNC) begins at the tips of the proximal tentacles and proceeds down these tentacles to their bases; soon after the proximal tentacles have begun to disintegrate, the process is initiated in the distal tentacles and proceeds to their bases. At about the same time as the tips of distal tentacles begin to die, the mouth region disintegrates and the disintegration extends along the body of the hydranth to its base. In many cases, it was observed that the outer surface of the proximal tentacles (i.e., the surface that contains the most nematocysts) preceded in disintegration the inner surface. In the stalk of the hydranth there is a specialized region bearing a ring of nematocysts; in many cases it was noted that this region disintegrated early and without any definite relation to the progress of disintegration in other parts of the hydranths, as was also noted by Child. Such specialized regions, owing to func- tional activity, are commonly highly susceptible to toxic agents, THE AXIAL GRADIENTS IN HYDROZOA. 365 a fact that has been noticed by us on many different forms. It is to be understood that the disintegration of the ectoderm precedes that of the entoderm by a considerable time interval except in the case of the rim of the mouth where both germ layers seem to disintegrate almost simultaneously forming a great bulging mass of disintegrated particles. The remarks in the foregoing paragraph refer to fully developed hydranths. In young hydranths, it was commonly observed that the distal tentacles disintegrated first and the disintegration then extended basipetally along the body of the hydranth, the. proximal tentacles disintegrating later than the body of the hydranth. In medusa-buds, the disintegration proceeds from the free to the attached end. Young hydranths are more susceptible than fully developed ones only after they have reached a certain stage. The hydranths are much more susceptible than the stems. The disintegration of the stems is obscured by the presence of the perisarc. Nevertheless the progress of disintegration was ob- served in many cases in the stems but was never followed for more than a short distance. The disintegration proceeds from the base of the hydranth down the stem. It might be said in criticism that the killing agent can gain access to the stem only from the open top of the perisarc and that the death gradient in the stem may therefore be simply a consequence of the diffusion path followed by the agent. To answer this objection short apical pieces of stems bearing hydranths were cut off and their disintegration followed in cyanide. In such cases the open cut surface at the proximal end of the stem affords a readier point of entrance for the reagent than does the top of the perisarc; nevertheless except for a small area of disintegration around the cut surface, the disintegration proceeds in the same manner as before, from the distal end of the stem proximally. Direct observation of the course of disintegration in the stems is not, however, entirely satisfying, as the process is admittedly obscured by the presence of the perisarc and could be observed for only the most distal region of the stems. Another method was therefore employed, namely, the differential survival of distal and proximal pieces of the stem in nearly lethal concentrations of toxic substances. The substance employed for this purpose 366 LIBBIE H. HYMAN. was ether. It was found that distal pieces are more susceptible to ether than proximal pieces, that more of them die when both kinds of pieces are exposed for a certain length of time to a given concentration, and that of those that survive, the regeneration is more delayed in the apical than in the-drotal halves. An experi- ment of this kind is recorded in Table I. In this experiment the TABLE I. DIFFERENTIAL SUSCEPTIBILITY OF APICAL AND BASAL PIECES OF EQUAL LENGTH (10-12 MM.) TO 2 PER CENT. ETHER. Exposed to ether for twenty hours. Table records condition of the pieces seven days after cutting. Temp. 12 ± 2° C. Condition. Apical. Basal. No. of hydranths emerged 5 14 No. of pieces with primordia of hydranth1 5 7 No. of pieces living but without primordia1 2 15 No. of pieces dead1 38 14 hydranths and first millimeter or two of the stems were dis- carded, the stems were then cut into two equal pieces, each 10-12 mm. long, and all of the distal pieces placed in one finger bowl and the proximal in another. A solution of 2 per cent, ether in sea-water was then poured on both sets of pieces as soon as possible after they were prepared and they were left in this for twenty hours. They were then thoroughly washed and left in normal sea-water. The temperature throughout was 12° C. ± 2. The condition of the fifty pairs of pieces seven days after cutting is shown in the table. The greater susceptibility of the apical pieces is perfectly evident. More of them have died than in the case of the basal pieces and of those that survived, the basal pieces are much in advance in the process of regeneration. C. GRADIENTS IN REDUCTION OF POTASSIUM PERMANGANATE. Child (190) has called attention to a new method of demon- strating the metabolic gradients. This method consists in exposing the organisms to appropriate concentrations of a readily reducible substance like potassium permanganate. This sub- stance is reduced by protoplasm, a brown precipitate of manga- 1 Pieces were examined with the compound microscope. Pieces showing smooth munded ends and circulation were counted as living, while stems containing only masses of granules were regarded as dead. THE AXIAL GRADIENTS IN HYDROZOA. 367 nese dioxide being formed. It was found by Child that this capacity of organisms to reduce permanganate exhibits the same kind of a gradation in relation to the body axes as does their time of death in toxic solutions. The apical ends reduce the most permanganate and take on a very deep brown or almost black color and the depth of this color decreases basipetally. I have observed the staining of Tubidaria by potassium per- manganate. The picture thus presented is identical with the course of disintegration already described. The tips of the proximal tentacles stain first, the tips of the distal tentacles next; the stain progresses rapidly down the tentacles (on the outer surfaces of the proximal tentacles before their inner surfaces) to their bases. Meantime the stain appears on the distal end of the body of the hydranth and progresses basipetally along its ectoderm. After the staining is completed, it is found that the tips of both sets of tentacles and the mouth region of the hydranth are very deeply stained and the stain shades off to the bases of tentacles and hydranth. This was best observed by turning the hydranth with a needle so that it faced upward. The staining of the stem was naturally difficult to observe. The short stalk below the hydranth became stained soon after the hydranth but the staining of the stem proper was very slow. As far as could be observed the stain proceeded from the distal end of the stem proximally. The observations were, however, unsatisfactory. The younger hydranths stain much more rapidly than the larger ones, but exhibit less distinct graded differences between different regions of the hydranth. D. ELECTRICAL GRADIENTS IN TUBULARIA. Differences in electrical potential along the axes of organisms form still another manifestation of the metabolic gradients and are at present being investigated by members of this laboratory for a number of the lower forms. These electrical gradients correspond in all respects to the death and staining gradients. The regions of highest susceptibility and reducing power are electronegative (galvanometrically) to regions of lower suscepti- bility and reducing power. This matter has been discussed 368 LIBBIE H. HYMAX. elsewhere (Hyman, '18) and further papers upon the subject will appear shortly. Briefly it is believed that chemical differences are as a rule responsible for these permanent differences in potential and that such chemical differences arise in the final analysis through differences in metabolic rate at different levels. The existence of such a difference in electrical potential along the axis of Tubularia was discovered by Mathews ('03). Mathews found that the hydranth is negative to any region of the stem and that distal levels of the stem are negative to proximal levels. He correctly attributed these potential differ- ences to differences in metabolic activity. I have repeated these experiments on Tubularia and verified all of Mathews' statements. A galvanometer constructed on the principle of the D'Arsonval galvanometer and put out by the Leeds and Northrup Company was used. Although not as sensitive as some other types of galvanometers in use, the instrument was found adequate for the purpose. Non-polariz- able electrodes were employed, made in the usual way of the glass tubes from medicine droppers, packed at the small end with kaolin paste made with sea-water, and filled at the other end with saturated zinc sulphate solution. Zinc rods, amalgamated with mercury, dipped into the zinc sulphate solution, and were con- nected by copper wires with the binding posts of the galvanom- eter. Small rolls of hard filter paper were thrust into the ends of the electrodes and the stems to be tested placed on these. These filter paper rolls \vere kept soaked with sea-water. Al- though such electrodes are made as nearly alike as possible, there is almost always some difference of potential between them. Such difference increased with use so that it was necessary to renew parts of the electrodes or to make wholly new ones at frequent intervals. The existence of a potential difference be- tween the two electrodes of course makes it impossible to obtain an absolute value of the amount of current originating from the organism; but absolute values were not desired in the present experiments. It was my purpose merely to discover which regions of the organism were electronegative as compared with other regions. In performing the experiments isolated stems of Tubularia in THE AXIAL GRADIENTS IN HYDROZOA. 369 perfect condition only were employed. They were placed across the filter paper ends of the electrodes and the reading of the galvanometer recorded. The stem was then reversed on the electrodes and the reading again taken. The difference in the two readings, particularly the difference in the direction of swing of the indicator on the scale gives the desired information about the electrical condition of the stems. Each reading was repeated once, sometimes twice. When dead organisms are tested in this way, the galvanometer gives the same reading regardless of the position of the material on the electrodes. The galvanometric readings obtained on Tiibularia are recorded in Table II. In connection with this table it is necessary to explain that the scale of the galvanometer used is printed in red on the right side of the zero point and in black on the left side. When the right electrode, which is connected with the right hand binding post of the galvanometer, is negative, the indicator swings to the right of the zero point and hence reads on the red half of the scale; when the left electrode is negative, the indicator reads on the left hand or black side of the scale. In some cases, both readings may be on the same side of the zero point but one farther to the right or left than the other. Left and right refer, of course, to the hands of the observer as he sits facing the instrument. In the table, each number refers to one individual and all of the data given under that number were obtained on one indi- vidual. In Table II., a, are recorded the readings of the gal- vanometer when the hydranth is compared with nearby portions of the stem or with distant portions or with regions where branches are present. Table II., b, gives the readings when distal portions of the stem are compared with more proximal regions or with far proximal regions or with proximal regions bearing branches. In each table, the first column gives the number of the individual, the second describes the material, the third gives the reading in one position between regions not very far separated, the fourth column the readings for the same regions in the reversed position on the electrodes, the fifth and sixth are the same as the third and fourth except that they compare regions more widely separated or distal regions with levels bearing branches. 370 LIBBIE H. HYMAN. TABLE II., a. ELECTRICAL GRADIENTS OF Tubularla; HYDRANTH COMPARED WITH NEARBY PORTIONS OF THE STEM AND WITH FAR PROXIMAL PORTIONS OR REGIONS BEAR- ING BRANCHES. Each number refers to one individual. Scale of the galvanometer reads with red figures to the right of the zero point, with black figures to the left of zero, hence r means red, and b means black, and numbers accompanying r and b are divisions on the scale. The right electrode is negative when the reading is on red or farther to the right than before; and the left electrode is negative when the reading is on black or farther to the left than before. Hydr., hydranth, br., region of branches, prox., proximal, dist., distal. No. Material. Readings with Reference to Position of Material on Electrodes as Designated in These Columns. Hydr. Right. Stem Left. Hydr. Left, Stem Right. Hydr. Hydr. Left, Right, Far Far Prox. Prox. or Br. or Br. Zone Zone Left. Right. I. 2. 3- 5- 6. S. 10. ii. 12. 13- 14. 15- 16. 17- 18. 19. 21. 23- 35- Hvdr. and stem .... 5 r 3 r 2 b 2 b o 2 r 2 r 0 4 r 5 r o 0 2 r 2 r o 2 b 3 r o 0 0 0 i r 2 b o 5 r o o i r 13 r 3 r 5 r 0 6 r Ib lb 4b 2 b 15 & 10 b 5b 5b 8 b 8 b 3b 5b 6 b 9b 5b 6 b 6 b 5b 4b 4b 6 b 4b 4b 2 b 2 b 6 b 3b 2 b 18 b 4b 4b 14 b I5j> 0 3 r o o 2 r 2 r 0 0 0 o 2 b 2 b o i r i b o 2 r 2 r 8 b 5b 4b 4b 6 b 6 b 2 b 2 b 5b 5b o o 5b 5b 3* 3 b 5b 2 b Small hvdr. and stem Hvdr., stem, and br. . Ditto Large hvdr., stem and br Small hvdr. and stem Large hvdr , stem and br Large hvdr. and stem Medium hvdr. and stem Ditto. . . Ditto Ditto, and br Ditto . Small hvdr. and stem Medium hydr stem and br Small hvdr and stem Very large hydr and stem . ATedium hvdr and ^tem Ditto THE AXIAL GRADIENTS IN HYDROZOA. 371 TABLE II., a, Continued. No. Material. Readings with Reference to Position of Material on Electrodes as Designated in these Columns. Hydr. Rieht, Stem ~Left. Hydr. Left, Stem Right. Hydr. Right. Far Prox. or Br. Zone Left. Hydr. Left. Far Prox. or Br. Zone Right. 37- 38. 39- 40. Ditto, and br 7 r 4 r 13 r 5 r 7 r 8 r 19 b 10 b i r o o 18 b 2 r 2 b o o r 6 b 2 b i b 6 b Medium hvdr. and stem Ditto, and br Medium hvdr. and stem.. The tables give all of the readings which were made. There have been no omissions or selection of data. Forty-two indi- viduals in all wrere tested. The first forty of these came from the same lot of Tubularia, collected on December 6, and tested on December 7 and 8. The last two individuals came from another lot of material collected on December 8 and tested on the same day. Material was kept at a temperature of 10° C. A number of different readings were commonly made on each stem, various levels of the stem being tested in order to obtain a picture of the potential differences along the whole organism. The following conclusions may be drawn from the data pre- sented in Tables II., a, and b: 1. The hydranth is always electronegative to nearby regions of the stem. This is shown without exception in the twenty- three cases given in Table II., a. The galvanometer invariably reads to the right when the hydranth is on the right electrode, and to the left when the position is reversed. 2. The difference between hydranth and distal regions of the stem is greater in the case of larger hydranths and much less in the case of small hydranths. Thus in nos. 2, 8, 17, and 19, where small hydranths were used, the potential difference be- tween hydranths and stem is 2 to 5 points of the scale; while when medium or large hydranths are used, the differences are much greater. These conditions are probably associated with age. 3. Hydranths are usually more negative to distal portions of the stem than to more proximal regions or regions where lateral 372 LIBBIE H. HYMAN. TABLE II., b. ELECTRICAL GRADIENTS OF Ttibularia. Distal regions of stem compared with proximal or with far proximal or region bearing branches. Otherwise as in table II., a. No. Material. Readings with Reference to Position of Material on Electrodes as Designated in These Columns. Dist. Stem Right, Prox. Left. Dist. Stem Left, Prox. Right. Dist. Stem Dist. Stem Right, Far Left, Far Prox. or Prox. or Br. Left. Br. Right. I. 4- 7- 9- 13- 20. 22. 24- 25- 26. 27. 28. 29. 30. 31- 32- 33- 34- 36. 41. 42. Hydr., stem and br i b i b i b o 0 i r o 4b 5 b 3 b 2 b 2 b 4b 3 b 3 b 2 b i b i b 2 r No potent 2 r 2 b I b 0 0 0 o i r i r No potent 12 r 12 r 7 r 8 r 5^ 5 b Sb 3b 3b 5b 3-Sb 2 r o 5 b 10 b 8 b 5b 4b 5b 5b 3b 3b 4b ial differem i b 4b 2 b 3b 2 b 2 b 3 b 2 b 4b ial differem o 5?> 9b Qb 2 b 2 b 0 I b 2 b 2 b 2 b 2 b :e i b i b :e 0 o 4b 4b 0 i b 0 0 i b I b i b i b 2 r 2 r Stem without hydr., hydr. regen- erating . Stem without hydr., like 4 Stem without hvdr., like 4. Medium hvdr and stem Ditto Ditto Ditto Ditto and br . Medium hydr. and stem Ditto . ... Ditto Ditto Ditto Ditto Ditto Ditto, and br Small hydr and stem . . Medium hydr stem and br.,. Hydr., very long stem Like 41.. THE AXIAL GRADIENTS IN HYDROZOA. 373 branches are present. This is shown in numbers 3, 5, 6, 8, 10, 15, 16, 18, 37, and 40, Table II., a. Thus for example, in no. 3, the difference between the hydranth and the distal stem is 15 divisions of the scale in the first trial, 12 in the second, while the difference between the same hydranth and a more proximal region of the stem where a lateral branch was present was 8 divisions in both trials. In one case, no. 13, the region of branching was negative to the hydranth; in another case, no. 39, there was practically no potential difference between the hydranth and the branching region. We therefore see that proximal regions of the stem are more negative than distal, especially when they bear branches. This is due to the fact that the hydranth domi- nates only a certain length of stem, and beyond that length physiological isolation has occurred with the formation of a new individual, expressed by the development of lateral branches. Such new individuals like the original one are electrically negative apically. 4. Distal regions of the stem are nearly always electronegative to nearby proximal regions. This was the case in 14 of the 17 cases tested in Table II., b. In two cases, nos. 28 and 34, there was no potential difference between two such regions of the stem; in one case, no. 13, the gradient was reversed, the distal region being positive to the proximal region. Such cases as these three account for the fact that occasionally, distal and proximal pieces of the stem regenerate simultaneously, or that the proximal piece may precede. 5. The potential difference between distal and proximal regions of the stem is always very much less than that between hydranth and distal regions of the stem. 6. The potential difference is usually slight or absent or may be reversed between distal regions of the stem and far proximal regions, or regions bearing branches. Of six cases tested, two showed no potential difference (nos. 4 and 33, Table II., b); in one case, the distal region was negative (no. i); and in the other three cases, the far proximal region or branching region was negative to the distal region (nos. 9, 25, 36). This verifies what was said in paragraph 3. These far proximal regions are really beginnings of new individuals and hence are more electro- negative than the regions immediately distal to them. 374 LIBBIE H. HYMAN. I therefore find, as Mathews did, that within the limits of a single Tubularia individual, any distal region is electronegative (galvanometrically) to any proximal region. Since electro- negativity is usually associated with a higher rate of oxidative metabolism in organisms, these experimental data constitute strong evidence that there is a metabolic gradient along the axis of Tubularia, that the apical end of this axis has the highest rate of activity, and that this rate diminishes proximally. E. DIFFERENCES IN RATE OF REGENERATION OF DISTAL AND PROXIMAL PIECES OF EQUAL LENGTH. A large number of experiments were performed with reference to this point with the result that the apical pieces were found to regenerate markedly faster than the basal pieces in practically all cases. A few cases were observed in which the proximal piece regenerated first. i. Method of Procedure. — The method of cutting the pieces was invariably as follows unless specifically stated otherwise. Stems free from branches and filled with coenosarc throughout their length were removed from the colony and placed on a glass plate. The hydranth and the first millimeter or two of the stem were then removed by a cut and discarded and the basal end injured by removal from the colony also cut off and discarded. The piece of stem was then cut into two equal halves, a distal or apical half and a proximal or basal half. In most cases, unless otherwise stated, such halves were 8-12 mm. long. Figure I illustrates the method of cutting the pieces. After cutting the pieces were handled in two different ways. In the majority of the experiments all of the apical halves were placed in one finger bowl and all of the basal halves in another finger bowl. Such experiments are designated throughout this paper as mass experiments. The number of oral hydranths emerged in each finger bowl at a given time was then recorded. In other experiments, which are designated as individual experi- ments, each half was placed in a separate stender dish and the time of emergence of the oral hydranth on each half stem recorded as accurately as possible. In all cases the record was taken only when the hydranth had emerged completely from the perisarc. THE AXIAL GRADIENTS IN HYDROZOA. 3/5 Observations were made and the hydranths emerged recorded every two to four hours during the daytime. No observations were made during the night and hence there are in all of the experiments gaps of from six to ten hours for each night period. The first morning observation after such a gap is indicated in all of the tables by an asterisk. There has been no selection of experiments for presentation in this paper. Practically all of the experiments performed are presented. 2. Mass Experiments in June. — These experiments were per- formed between June 16 and July 9, 1919. The material was in excellent condition up to July i, when the pieces for the last experiment were cut. There was a great abundance of material, growing rapidly and containing hydranths of all sizes. Material was always cut on the same day as collected since, as is well known, Tubularia will not keep in good condition in the labora- tory in the summer. The hydranths fall off within twenty-four fours, new ones being subsequently regenerated; further the ccenosarc either dies away in the basal regions or else retreats to other parts of the colonies. The regenerating pieces were kept in the laboratory during the earlier experiments; the tempera- ture was naturally variable and as recorded in the daytime ranged from 15° to 24° C., with probably lower temperatures at night. The later experiments were placed in the refrigerator at a constant temperature of 13° C. The mass experiments performed in June are recorded in Table III. As already stated, all of the apical halves were placed in one finger bowl and all of the basal halves in another; the two finger bowls were kept under the same conditions. At frequent intervals the regenerating pieces were examined and the number of oral hydranths emerged recorded. The record was taken only when the hydranth had completely emerged from the top of the ccenosarc and had spread its tentacles. Details of these experiments not given in the table are as follows. Experiment I was performed on slender stems; experi- ments 2 on stout stems; the other experiments were with medium sized stems although there was some variation in the diameter of the stems. Experiments I, 2, 5, and 10 regenerated in the 376 LIBBIE H. HYMAX. TABLE III. fa o & fa o 71 H J < ffi J «! w r^cooO'COco o\O\C\O O M M M 01 -rj-io S c = p * CJ -i d; "£ ^ C 0 M M o] 0' M M 10 10 i- o. ^ r- 0, Cs 0 M M M ^ *t rt o r- ON O !_' (N (N (N 01 rorOf^f^O^^O^^roro P (n °co ce a d rcr^-ON^OGO cs ONO IDO r^-oo M M r^iooo o «j (_> " UOl^lOlOlOlOlO LOj-U^lOtOi/^ LOUO C.~ "o .5 CO k^ 2 a ^^^.O.O^o 0000 ^^.= =00,0 aov0,o 0 « n 'S 000-=l-xOvOONOOOM(N(n H 01 CS CN CN •sl « l-H M' M M O) N 0) 01 umbers cated. en i- a '* * -- g.S ^- 000000, 00.0=0 c^^^,,^.. 2 P a -a S M HH M CS CS C^ CS c/j +-> n% • E? IO in a ^^ ^tt^ # ^ M cu P £ O " ^ CJ ^ 1— 1 HH Ol N gl « HH M M M CS C P 0 | 7 >> oo .a uJ a UO IO #° * CO i—, 0 0 o >- ^ M 1— 1 M p. . f"~l 5 « in IT; j>. 3 C o '5 i en u LO UO LO f^* O* O C1) c*; LO t^* M t— i *° ^ -jT THE AXIAL GRADIENTS IN HYDROZOA. 377 laboratory at variable and generally moderately warm tempera- tures, from 15° to 24° C. (day records). In experiment 6, the pieces were placed in the refrigerator (temp. 13° C.) for the first twelve hours and in experiment n for the first twenty hours after section, and were then removed to laboratory temperature. In experiments 15, 16, and 21 the pieces were kept in the re- frigerator (temp. 13° C.) for the entire period of regeneration as the weather had become unfavorably warm by this time. There was some mortality, particularly among the basal pieces, owing probably to the warm weather. In experiment 6, two basal pieces were living but had not regenerated when the experiment was concluded. In experiment 10, where fifty pairs of pieces were cut, seven basal pieces died and four had failed to regenerate when the experiment was discontinued. In experi- ment 1 1 , three apical halves and twelve basal halves had died or failed to regenerate when the experiment was discontinued. One basal piece died in experiment 21. The length of the pieces in all of the experiments recorded in Table III. was 8-12 mm. It was not possible to find stems free from branches long enough to give longer pieces in the summer material, owing to the fact that the colonies are growing rapidly and branching extensively at this season. The number of pieces cut in each experiment depended on the number of healthy stems of sufficient length available in the day's collection. Although material was very abundant and large quantities of it were brought in whenever desired, most of the colonies consisted of stems so short as to be useless for the experiments. 3. Mass Experiments in December. — According to the state- ments of Mr. Gray, head of the supply department at Woods Hole, Tubularia is most abundant and in excellent condition in the early summer reaching a climax in June. After that, as the weather becomes warm, the colonies die away, the protoplasm withdrawing into the perisarc and apparently passing into a dormant state. In the fall, as the water becomes colder, the colonies begin to grow again, reaching their height in November and December, and then with still colder weather, once more passing into the quiescent state, emerging in the spring. In November, 1919, no Tubularia could be found at Woods Hole, 378 LIBBIE H. HYMAN. in spite of diligent search by the collectors. It was, however, obtainable early in December in fair abundance, and experiments were performed upon it from December 6 to 16. The colonies at this time were in excellent condition, branching freely and growing rapidly. The general appearance of the material was much the same as in June except that the hydranths attained a larger size than in June and a few lots of material consisted of very long stems, much longer than any observed in June. The majority of the December material, however, was branching so freely that most of the stems were relatively short and in some cases it was necessary to cut pieces less than 8 mm. long. The temperature of the running water in the laboratory in December was 8° C. and it was therefore possible to keep the material for two or three days in excellent condition. Two collections of material were used in the December experiments; one collected on December 6 was cut for experiments on December 6, 7, and 8; the other, collected on December 8, was used on December 8 to 1 1 . All of the pieces were kept on the water tables in slowly running water at a temperature of approximately 12° C., varying, however, for slight periods from 10° to 14°. The results of the mass experiments performed in December are given in Table IV. In experiment 26, the pieces were about 5 mm. long; in experiment 27, 5-8 mm. long; and in experi- ments 35, 44, and 47, 10—12 mm. long. The pieces were cut as in Fig. I, except in the case of experiment 44, in which the basal pieces were cut at the proximal end of long stems so that some 10-15 mm. of stem was removed between the levels of the apical and basal pieces in this experiment. There was no mortality among the pieces. The temperature throughout was 12° C. ±2. 4. Conclusions from Mass Experiments. — The data given in Tables III. and IV. permit us to draw the following conclusions concerning the rate of regeneration of oral hydranths on apical and basal pieces of the stem of Tubularia of equal length : (a) Hydranths invariably emerge first on the apical pieces and a considerable number of such pieces will have regenerated before any of the basal pieces have produced a hydranth. (b} At any given time there are in nearly all cases a greater THE AXIAL GRADIENTS IN HYDROZOA. 379 number of apical pieces with oral hydranths than basal pieces. The difference is always more marked in the early part of the regeneration period; later the basal pieces may catch up with the more tardy of the apical pieces with the result that the number of regenerated basal pieces may in a few cases equal the TABLE IV. MASS EXPERIMENTS PERFORMED IN DECEMBER SHOWING RATE OF REGENERATION OF APICAL AND BASAL HALVES OF STEMS OF Tubnlaria. Hrs. means number of hours elapsed since cutting; a, apical half; b, basal half; figures under a and b give number of hydranths emerged at time indicated; asterisk indicates first morning observation. 26. 27. 35- 44- 47- Hrs. n. b. Hrs. a. b. Hrs. a. b. Hrs. a. b. Hrs. a. b. *6i 18 7 45 I o *6o 16 0 52 2 0 6l I o 63 19 9 Si 2 o 62 19 o 58 4 0 65 2 o 65 21 ii 53 8 2 64 22 4 61 8 o 67 4 o 67 21 14 55 15 3 66 23 9 *69 16 I 69 5 o 69 22 15 57 25 8 68 29 18 7i 18 8 73 12 3 7i 17 59 27 19 70 31 25 73 19 10 75 14 3 73 18 *68 40 40 72 35 29 75 22 16 *85 26 14 76 18 70 42 42 74 37 29 77 25 19 87 29 18 *84 20 72 48 44 77 4i 30 79 26 20 89 32 20 90 21 74 49 45 *S4 44 36 81 27 23 9i 33 26 *II2 22 76 5i 46 87 45 42 83 28 25 95 33 30 80 53 50 89 46 45 *93 30 27 97 34 31 83 54 5i 9i 47 47 105 28 99 35 33 *92 55 55 93 49 47 *ii7 29 101 36 33 94 57 56 95 49 49 119 30 *in 37 36 96 57 57 97 5i 49 119 37 98 58 57 99 5i IOO 60 57 104 58 *ii7 60 number of regenerated apical pieces (exps. 27 and 35, Table IV.) ; but in no case are the basal pieces in advance. (c) In all cases the apical pieces complete their regeneration first. (d) When other factors are equal the rate of regeneration is a function of temperature. (e) When other factors are equal, the rate of regeneration is a function of the diameter of the stem. More slender stems re- generate more rapidly than stouter stems. Thus in experiment I, Table III., the pieces were cut from slender stems bearing small hydranths; those in experiment 2, same table, from stouter 380 LIBBIE H. HYMAN. stems bearing larger hydranths, cut at the same time and from the same lot of material. It is perfectly apparent that the more slender pieces regenerate more rapidly, and this was also evi- denced throughout all of my experiments. It is probable that this relation of the rate of regeneration to the diameter of the stem is connected with the age of the stem, but since one does not certainly know that slender stems are younger than stouter ones, the matter must be left open at present. Morgan ('06 6) found that young stems regenerate more rapidly than old ones and that when the hydranths are removed from the top and lateral branches of a stem, the lateral branches regenerate first. At any rate, these facts dispose of the suggestion which has been made that apical pieces regenerate more rapidly than basal pieces because they are of larger diameter and hence contain more protoplasm. As a matter of fact it is the pieces of smaller diameter which regenerate the more rapidly. Further in slender stems there is no difference in diameter along the stem, and yet the apical halves of such stems regenerate hydranths earlier than the basal halves. 5. Individual Experiments. — These experiments were identical with the mass experiments except that each piece was placed in a separate dish and the number of hours required for it to produce an oral hydranth recorded as accurately as possible. The records of the four experiments of this kind which were performed are given in Table V. Experiments 9 and 17 were performed in June at room temperatures; experiments 29 and 45 in December at a temperature of 12° C. ± 2. Pieces were 8-12 mm. long except in experiment 29, where they were 5-8 mm, long. There was some mortality in the June experiments but none in December. 6. Conclusions from Individual Experiments. — The results of these experiments lead to the same conclusions as previously stated from mass experiments. Of 122 pairs of pieces in which both pieces regenerated, the apical halves regenerated hydranths first in in cases, or 91 per cent.; the basal halves first in 10 cases, or 8 per cent.; and the time of emergence of the hydranth was practically the same in both pieces in one case. Cases where the basal piece preceded in regeneration are indicated by THE AXIAL GRADIENTS IN HYDROZOA. 381 TABLE V. RECORDS OF THE TIME OF EMERGENCE OF INDIVIDUAL APICAL AND BASAL HALVES OF THE SAME STEM. Dagger calls attention to cases where the basal half emerged first; other abbre- viations and symbols as before. No. Hours Since Cutting. No. Hours Since Cutting. No. Hours Since Cutting. No. Hours Since Cutting. Exp. 9. Exp. 17. Exp. 29. Exp. 45. a. b. a. b. a. b. a. b. I 43 dead I 40 *64 I 45 71 I 73 *9i 2 *34 *5S 2 77 86 2 103 *H4 2 *67 77 3 42 75 3 *39 *64 3 65 79 3 60 75 4 44 50 4 *39 53 4 *88 79t 4 73 77 5 41 dead 5 43 77 5 57 73 5 60 73 6 42 dead 6 *39 45 6 56 69 6 *9I 73t 7 38 48 7 *39 76 7 54 7i 7 79 *67t 8 42 61 8 45 67 8 75 *88 8 59 71 9 38 44 9 68 67t 9 *64 66 9 56 *67 10 40 50 10 *64 74 10 *64 72 10 59 71 ii 38 50 1 1 *64 dead ii 56 74 ii *H5 93t 12 42 48 12 70 dead 12 56 76 12 *67 95 13 46 50 13 71 *87 13 53 *64 13 72 74 14 42 48 14 45 *&4 14 *64 *8? 14 72 73 15 36 50 15 4 1 53 15 66 *87 15 *66 *H4 16 42 50 16 A I dead 16 *64 68 16 *90 78t 17 36 46 17 dead 116 17 68 90 17 *66 74 18 53 dead 18 75 112 18 92 97 18 72 78 19 42 67 Aver. 53 73 19 66 *87 19 68 76 20 36 46 20 *64 70 20 *66 76 21 42 63 21 76 *87 21 72 76 22 36 46 22 70 96 22 68 78 23 42 46 23 70 96 23 68 80 24 38 40 24 68 *87 24 *66 76 26 42 44 25 *64 76 25 68 76 27 42 42 26 *64 72 26 68 *90 28 40 *58 27 68 *II2 27 68 72 29 42 63 28 90 95 28 78 74t 30 50 *58 29 73 104 29 68 78 31 48 63 30 66 76 30 58 70 32 65 67 31 72 *II2 31 *66 72 33 61 78 32 68 78 32 *66 76 34 63 62. st 33 114 99t 33 68 76 __35_ 40 46 34 69 *86 34 68 78 Aver. 42 52 35 *86 9i 35 72 76 36 69 75 36 *66 92 37 75 *86 Aver. 69 77 38 75 *86 39 67 *86 40 *86 101 41 66 97 42 118 I03t Aver. 7i 83 Total number of regenerated pairs . . . Number of cases where a preceded . . . Number of cases where b preceded . . . Number of cases where a and b equal . 122 in or 91% 10 or 8% i 382 LIBBIE H. HYMAN. a dagger in Table V. The average difference between the number of hours required for the emergence of the hydranths on apical and basal halves was 10 hours in exp. 9; 20 hours in exp. 17; 12 hours in exp. 29; and 8 hours in exp. 45. The individual differences range from half an hour to more than forty hours. It may be inquired why in a small percentage of cases the basal piece precedes the apical piece in regeneration, and why there is such a great variation in the difference between the time of regeneration of the two pieces. It is highly probable that these variations are related to the degree of physiological isolation existent in the basal pieces before they were cut from the stems. It has already been pointed out that a hydranth controls only a certain length of the stem proximal to it and beyond that limit a new individual arises which eventually expresses its presence by the formation of a lateral bud. Now it is evident that such new individuals must exist physiologically before they give morphological expression of their existence by bud formation. It has already been stated that in these experiments the longest obtainable stems free from buds were used. Such stems are the exception rather than the rule since the majority of the material obtainable, particularly in the summer, will furnish only a small proportion of long stems free from branches. It is therefore obvious that the basal regions of such long stems must be in various stages of the process of physiological isolation and branch formation. The nearer such basal regions are to branch formation the more rapidly will they regenerate when isolated and those that are on the very verge of branch formation may conceivably regenerate as rapidly as or even more rapidly than more apical pieces. This matter is referred to again in connec- tion with experiments on the rate of regeneration of basal pieces cut below branches. 7. Remarks on the Temperature Coefficient. — It has generally been accepted that when the rate of a biological process increases two to three times with each ten degrees rise in temperature that such a process is chemical in nature. It may be doubted that this line of reasoning is strictly correct. The use of the temperature coefficient to analyze the nature of a biological process involves the unwarranted assumption that such processes THE AXIAL GRADIENTS IN HYDROZOA. 383 may be purely chemical; but it is very doubtful that they ever are solely chemical in nature, and, of course, equally doubtful that they are ever the consequence of purely physical changes- In all probability biological processes are neither complexes of purely chemical reactions nor purely the resultants of physical changes but they involve both types of changes occurring simul- taneously and mutually interacting. On a priori grounds, how- ever, it may be accepted that the chemical processes are of paramount importance in living things, since, while substances having physical properties similar to or identical with those of protoplasm exist which are not alive, in no case do non-living materials carry on the chemical reactions characteristic of proto- plasm; further, the "signs of life" are chemical or of chemical origin, and protoplasm in which the chemical reactions have fallen to a low level is to all intents dead. Granting, therefore, that chemical reactions play the most important roles in life processes and that in many cases physical changes are insignifi- cant, it may be valid to draw conclusions from the value of the temperature coefficient. But it must always be borne in mind that the chemical reactions which occur in living things are subject to processes of regulation in the organism. The relation of chemical changes to temperature is therefore in the organism a variable quantity. Thus Behre ('18) found that the rate of respiratory metabolism of Planaria is lowered when the animals are maintained at a high temperature and raised when they are maintained at low temperatures. The temperature coefficient for the rate of respiration of Planaria is therefore not a fixed value for a certain range of temperature but depends to some extent upon the temperature at which the animals had been living previous to the experimental test. Since such modifica- tions or regulations are known for emulsoid colloids, their be- havior at any given time depending upon the conditions to which they had previously been exposed (phenomenon of hys- teresis), it is possible that this ability of organisms to modify the rate of processes presumably chiefly chemical with reference to temperature is due to the colloidal substratum in which the chemical reactions take place. In Tubularia similar regulations to temperature are observable. 384 LIBBIE H. HYMAX. The rate of regeneration of pieces of the stem of Tubularia is, as has long been known, dependent in large part upon the tem- perature and the temperature coefficient of this process is described as corresponding to that of chemical reasions (Moore, '10). The rate of regeneration is not, however, wholly dependent upon the temperature at which regeneration occurs but is to some degree affected by the temperature at which the particular stems used had been living previous to their utilization. Thus in the experiments recorded in Tables III. and IV., it can be noted that summer material regenerates more slowly at 13° C. than does winter material at 12° C. While other possible explanations of this fact could be suggested it seems reasonable in the light of other results along this line to suppose that this is another case of acclimation to temperature; material living for some time at low temperature has elevated the rate of its chemical processes above that which would result if the material were suddenly lowered to the same temperature from a higher tem- perature— a procedure usually practised in experiments on the temperature coefficient. F. RATE OF REGENERATION OF DISTAL AND PROXIMAL PIECES OF UNEQUAL LENGTH. Banus refers to Child's experiments on pieces of unequal length in which Child found that longer pieces will regenerate slightly faster than shorter ones provided the factor of level is eliminated by always making the apical pieces the shorter pieces. Since apical pieces regenerate faster than basal pieces no con- clusions could be drawn regarding the effect of length on the time of regeneration unless the apical piece were the shorter. Banus has "repeated" this experiment and claims that the longer piece always regenerates first regardless of level. Here Banus has again misrepresented Child's statements and he has not in reality repeated Child's experiment. Child distinctly states that changes in the length of the piece "produce only very slight or no appreciable differences in time of emergence of oral hydranths provided the length of the piece is above a certain minimum. But with reduction in length below the minimum the appearance of the hydranth is delayed and this retardation increases with THE AXIAL GRADIENTS IN HYDROZOA. 385 further reduction in length." The apical pieces will therefore regenerate later than the basal pieces only when they are reduced below a certain minimum length. This minimum length is very much less than any used in Banus's experiments. In order to get the result mentioned by Child it is necessary to cut the apical pieces as small as 2 mm. Yet Banns in "repeating" Child's experiments has used no pieces less than 10 mm. in length. In pieces as long as this, length makes very little difference; according to Child's results and my own, the apical pieces will regenerate oral hydranths earlier than the basal pieces, just as when both pieces are of equal size. In his tables 3, 4, and 5, Banus presents data on apical and basal pieces in which the lengths of the pieces were: 10 and 20 mm., 10 and 30 mm., and 10 and 40 mm. Banus found that the longer pieces in all of these cases, regardless of whether they are apical or basal, regenerate oral hydranths slightly in advance of the shorter pieces. With these results and statements of Banus I am quite unable to agree. There is some truth in the statement that a longer piece will regenerate slightly faster than a shorter piece with apical end at the same level. Yet in the case of apical pieces, the difference between pieces 10 and 20 mm. long is very slight indeed, in fact, practically nil; but it is plainly marked in shorter pieces, say 5 and 10 mm. long. In the case of the basal pieces the difference in time of regeneration between 10 and 20 mm. pieces is somewhat greater but here it must be remembered that the apical end of a basal piece 20 mm. long is in these experi- ments at a level 10 mm. more distal than that of a basal piece 10 mm. long, and the factor of level again comes into play. In all cases in pieces exceeding 5 mm. in length, the apical pieces will in general regenerate more quickly than the basal pieces, regardless of their relative lengths; and a basal piece twice as long as an apical piece will still regenerate more slowly than the apical piece, notwithstanding the effect of length. I have repeated Banus's experiment on relatively long pieces of unequal length and the results are given in Table VI. Three pairs of experiments were performed. In one experiment*of each pair the apical piece was half as long as the basal; in the other 386 LIBBIE H. HYMAN. experiment, the apical piece was twice as long as the basal. Pieces for the two experiments of each pair were cut from the same lot of stems at the same time and kept under the same conditions. All of the apical pieces were kept in one finger bowl and the basal in another. The method of cutting the pieces for such experiments is given in text-figure 2. All of these experi- ments were performed in December at a temperature of 12° ± 2. The length of the pieces is stated in the table. No experiments were attempted with pieces in which the ratio of length was I : 3 or i : 4, as it is difficult if not impossible to obtain a sufficient number of unbranched stems of the requisite length. The data given in Table VI. show quite clearly that an TABLE VI. RECORDS OF MASS EXPERIMENTS WITH APICAL AND BASAL PIECES OF UNEQUAL LENGTH. Columns under a and b record number of hydranths emerged at time indicated. Exp. 32, a = 4 — 6 Mm., b = 8 — 12 Mm Exp. 33, a = 8 — 12 Mm., /; = 4 — 6 Mm. Exp. 37, a = 8 — 10 Mm., 6=15—20 Mm. Exp. 38. ^ = 15 — 20 Mm., £=8— -10 Mm. Exp. 42, « = io — ii Mm., b=2o — 22 Mm. Exp. 43, a=2o — 22 ftlm.. />=io — ii Mm, Mrs. a. b. Mrs. a. b. Hrs. a. /.. Hrs. a. b. Hrs. a. b. Hrs. a. b. 56 I O 58 I O *59 18 0 *59 7 o 53 3 o 53 7 o 58 2 o *6y 15 2 61 24 i 6l II O 55 7 o 55 9 o *67 II 9 69 16 4 63 35 3 63 i? 0 57 ii O 57 10 0 69 13 ii 7i 18 5 65 4i 7 65 22 0 59 18 0 59 12 I 7i 15 16 73 21 7 67 47 13 67 24 3 62 20 0 62 17 2 73 I? i? 75 22 U 69 49 18 69 28 10 *69 25 25 *69 26 18 75 2O 19 77 17 ?i 49 25 71 28 14 72 26 26 72 28 24 77 21 20 79 19 73 50 27 73 30 18 74 26 27 74 28 26 79 22 22 81 20 76 30 76 32 21 76 26 28 76 29 27 84 23 22 84 21 *83 35 *83 35 34 78 26 29 78 29 29 *9i 23 23 *9i 22 86 38 86 37 38 82 27 30 82 30 29 94 24 23 88 40 88 38 45 84 29 84 29 104 24 90 4i 90 43 46 *96 30 *96 30 94 43 94 43 47 96 44 96 44 49 98 46 *io8 49 SO *io8 47 114 50 no 48 120 49 *I32 50 G. ALTERATION OF THE REGIONAL DIFFERENCES IN RATE OF REGENERATION. apical piece will regenerate faster than ii basal piece of twice its length, in pieces at least as long as 5 mm. Although the factor of length is of some consequence, the factor of level is of vastly THE AXIAL GRADIENTS IN HYDROZOA. 387 greater importance. In experiments 32 and 33, the longer pieces in each case regenerate faster, but the effect of length does not overcome the effect of level, the apical pieces in both experiments regenerating first on the whole. The influence of length is most marked in experiments 32 and 33, where short pieces were employed. It is very little evident in experiments 37 and 38, and 42 and 43, where pieces 10 mm. in length were employed. In fact, in experiment 37, the apical pieces 10 mm. long regenerate slightly faster than the apical pieces 20 mm. long; and in experi- ments 42 and 43 there is practically no difference. We may therefore say that length is of little consequence in pieces exceed- ing 10 mm. in length, in agreement with Child's previous state- ment and in contradiction to the claims of Banus. In all experiments the longer basal pieces regenerate faster than the shorter basal pieces, but it is probable that this effect is one of level rather than of length, because the longer basal pieces have their apical ends at a higher level than the shorter basal pieces. It may therefore be concluded that in the case of apical and basal pieces of unequal length, the apical pieces will still regen- erate more rapidly on the whole regardless of whether they constitute the shorter or the longer pieces, always provided that their minimum length is 5 mm. The level at which the pieces are cut is still the dominant factor in such experiments. In pieces below 10 mm. in size, a longer piece will regenerate slightly faster than a shorter one; but in pieces above 10 mm. length, length is of practically no consequence. Long basal pieces in such experiments regenerate faster than shorter basal ones mainly because their apical ends are at a higher level than the apical ends of the shorter pieces. These results are the contrary of those of Banus whose experiments are invalidated owing to his erroneous method of cutting the apical pieces as discussed at greater length below. The data already presented incontestably demonstrate that a regional difference in rate of regeneration exists along the axis of Tubularia, such that regeneration is the more rapid the nearer the piece lies to the apical end. It may next be inquired whether this regional difference is modifiable under either certain normal conditions or under experimental conditions. To this inquiry 388 LIBBIE H. HYMAN. an affirmative answer may be returned. It is possible to modify or eliminate the regional differences in question. The various methods by means of which this was attempted or accomplished are discussed in this section. 1. The Effect of Cold. — A number of experiments were per- formed in which both apical and basal pieces were exposed to a lowered temperature for a number of hours after cutting or during the entire period of regeneration. The pieces were removed from room temperatures (approximately 20° C.) to the temperature of the refrigerator (13° C.) for various periods of time. Although such a proceeding invariably retards the rate of regeneration, the differences between the regeneration of apical and basal pieces were unaltered by such exposure to low tempera- ture. These experiments are therefore included in Table III. (exps. 6, n, 15, 1 6, and 21), as showing the typical difference between the rate of regeneration of apical and basal halves. It is highly probable, however, that with very low temperatures, in the neighborhood of zero, the typical difference between pieces of different level would be reduced or eliminated. 2. Effect of Using Material Kept in the Laboratory. — Two experiments were performed in June upon material which had been kept in the laboratory aquaria for a week preceding the cutting of the pieces. As is well known under such circum- stances, the hydranths of Tubularia fall from the stems and new hydranths are subsequently regenerated. Such new hydranths are smaller than and have a lower rate of activity than the original hydranths; it is therefore to be expected that the regional differences along the axis will be reduced in such cases. As already stated only two experiments were performed as the weather had become warm and little material was available. The material for these experiments was collected on June 30 and cut on July 6. The pieces were kept in the refrigerator (13° C.) throughout the regeneration period. The results are given in Table VII. Experiment 21, Table III., furnishes a control for these experiments. It will readily be seen that the differences between the rate of regeneration of apical and basal pieces are plainly reduced as a consequence of the depressing effect of laboratory conditions upon the physiological axis of THE AXIAL GRADIENTS IN HYDROZOA. 389 Tubularia. This experiment shows that the metabolic gradient of Tubularia is not a fixed and permanent gradient in the stem but is readily variable under the conditions of the animal's environment. Experiments such as those of Banus in which no account is taken nor any description given of the conditions of the material or the environment do not therefore merit serious consideration. TABLE VII. RECORD OF MASS EXPERIMENTS WITH APICAL AND BASAL PIECES OF EQUAL LENGTH, THE PIECES BEING TAKEN FROM MATERIAL KEPT ONE WEEK IN- LABORATORY CONDITIONS BEFORE CUTTING. Temp. 13° C. Control, cxp. 21, Table III., in which the pieces were cut on the same day as the material was collected. Exp. 24. Exp. 25. Hrs. a. b. Hrs. a. b. 72 I 0 72 4 O *84 II 5 *84 9 3 87 12 9 87 n 6 94 22 16 89 13 9 96 22 18 94 22 18 *io7 33 28 96 23 23 no 33 30 *i07 27 26 114 35 33 no 28 28 117 36 33 114 29 28 119 36 33 117 29 30 123 37 35 119 30 *I46 36 158 37 3. The Effect of the Presence of Branches. — In the consideration of the data on the electrical gradient in Tubularia it was pointed out that the control of a Tubularia hydranth extends only for a limited distance down the stem and that the stem beyond this limit is more or less differentiated as another individual. This differentiation, at first purely physiological, is later morpho- logically apparent by the formation of a bud at the level of the apical end of the new individual. The appearance of the bud not only indicates the formation of a new individual but also is an expression of a loss of control of the basal portions of the stem by the original hydranth. It is therefore to be expected that pieces taken above such lateral branches will have a lower metabolic rate than pieces of the same level from unbranched stems; and further that pieces taken below the branch, since 590 LIBBIE H. HYMAN. they are near the apical ends of new individuals (the real apical end being the hydranth of the branch) will have a higher meta- bolic rate than ordinary basal pieces. Owing to the operation of both of these factors it may be expected that the difference between apical and basal pieces will be reduced when they are cut above and below, respectively, the level of a branch. This was found to be the case. Banus in his paper does not state whether or not he used stems free from branches and did not reply to inquiries on this point. In preparing pieces for this kind of experiment, the following procedure was usually adopted. Stems having one branch at about the middle of the stem were selected, the terminal hy- dranth, upper millimeter or two, and basal end cut off and dis- carded as usual. An apical piece was then cut anterior to the branch, and a basal piece of equal length posterior to the branch; the small piece bearing the branch was discarded. As found by Morgan and verified in my experiments, the stumps of lateral branches left on pieces wTill regenerate hydranths more rapidly than the distal end of such pieces, and these lateral hydranths will then inhibit the formation of the terminal hydranth; hence in experiments of this kind it is necessary to avoid using pieces bearing stumps of branches. The method of cutting the pieces in most of the experiments is illustrated in Fig. 3. In one experiment, stems having two branches were selected, the apical piece cut in front of the first branch, and the basal piece between the two branches as illustrated in Fig. 4. The results are presented in Table VIII. All experiments of this kind were mass experiments. Experiments I2a and 126 were performed in June at room temperature, the remaining experiments in December at 12° C. ± 2. The controls for these experiments are indicated at the top of the table; such controls were cut at the same time and from the same lot of material, with the exception that they came from stems without branches, and were kept under the same conditions. The pieces in experi- ments 28, 30, and 34 were short pieces, 5-10 mm. long; those in experiments 12 and 36, approximately 10 mm. long. The pieces for all experiments except number 36 were prepared according to Fig. 3; those for experiment 36 as in Fig. 4. THE AXIAL GRADIENTS IN HYDROZOA. 391 The effect on the relative times of regeneration of apical and basal pieces by cutting them above and below a lateral branch is of course slight but nevertheless it is evident in most cases. If the experiments given in Table VIII. are compared with those TABLE VIII. RECORDS OF MASS EXPERIMENTS WITH APICAL AND BASAL PIECES OF EQUAL LENGTH, THE APICAL PIECES BEING TAKEN IN FRONT OF THE FIRST BRANCH THE BASAL PIECES BELOW THE BRANCH. Columns under a and b give number of hydranths emerged at time indicated. Controls in Table III. and IV., exp. 10 for exp. 12; exps. 26 and 27 for exp. 28, 30 and 34; exp. 35 for exp. 36. Exp. iza. Exp. i2/>. Exp. 28, Exp. 30. Exp. 34. Exp. 36. Hrs. a. b. Hrs. a. b. Hrs. a. b. Hrs. a. b. Hrs. it. b. Hrs. <*. b. 37 I O 33 I 0 53 I O 54 I 0 56 I 0 *59 2 O 43 2 0 37 2 0 55 2 O *62 4 0 58 3 0 63 4 O 45 3 O *43 6 2 57 3 0 66 ! 5 2 *67 17 2 65 5 0 49 4 I 47 10 3 60 n 0 68 7 4 69 17 3 67 8 2 5i 4 3 49 13 6 *68 13 5 70 9 8 7i 17 8 69 n 4 *57 6 4 5i 16 9 70 14 6 72 9 9 73 22 9 71 14 IO 60 8 4 53 17 13 72 IS 7 79 n 10 75 25 14 73 15 II 66 9 5 55 18 15 74 16 9 82 ii 13 77 32 20 76 18 12 68 10 6 59 25 19 76 17 n *90 12 13 79 35 26 *83 28 20 72 ii 7 61 28 20 78 18 12 93 13 14 81 38 27 86 35 23 74 n 8 *68 29 22 80 19 13 95 15 15 84 40 30 88 36 27 *8i ii 9 7i 30 24 83 20 , 15 99 15 16 *9i 55 41 90 43 31 88 12 10 75 3i 24 *QI 22 18 *ii6 16 94 57 41 92 47 36 95 13 10 77 25 94 19 96 59 42 94 48 40 105 14 10 85 25 117 20 98 44 96 48 41 108 1 1 *92 26 119 22 IOO 47 98 48 43 no 12 97 27 1 06 1 5I *io8 5i 46 IOO 28 *n6 58 no 48 107 29 122 59 114 49 I I2O Si in Tables III. and IV., it will be found that in general more basal pieces have regenerated in the experimental series when the same number of apical pieces have regenerated in both control and experimental series. This appears chiefly in the early part of the regeneration period. A few such comparisons may be pointed out; in making them it is necessary to select experiments in which the total number of regenerating pieces is similar in experiment and control, since the number of regenerated apical pieces in the early stages of an experiment is greater relative to the basal pieces, the greater the total number of pieces. In experiment I2&, Table VIII., 2, 9, and 19 basal pieces have 392 LIBBIE H. HYMAN. formed hydranths as compared with i, 6, and less than 15 basal pieces in the control experiment, number 10, Table III., when 6, 1 6, and 25 apical pieces have regenerated in both cases. Similarly, in experiment 30, for which experiment 26 is a control, the regeneration of the apical and basal pieces is practically simultaneous, a result which is never obtained when stems free from branches are employed. Comparison of experiment 36, Table VIII., with its control, experiment 35, Table IV., shows the same effect; in the former case 12 basal pieces, in the latter case no basal pieces have regenerated at the time when 18 apical pieces have regenerated in both experiments. In other cases, as in experiment 34, little difference from the control could be observed. The decrease in the time difference between the apical and basal pieces in these experiments is apparently largely due to a delay in the regeneration of the apical pieces. This is to be expected, since as already explained all basal pieces are probably more or less isolated as new individuals, and hence are slightly accelerated in both experimental and control series. The "apical" pieces, on the other hand, in the present experi- ments, since they are taken in front of the level of a branch, really represent the basal end of the first zooid of the stem, and hence are delayed in regeneration as compared with pieces similar in position from stems where branches have not yet arisen and where the hydranth still controls most of the length of the stem. In regard to the basal pieces, it should further be pointed out that the really high metabolic point of the new individuals formed at the base of Tiibularia stems is in the hydranth of the branch, and the basal piece itself below the level of the branch retains only part of the increased metabolic rate after the bud has formed. Banus has presented one table in which he has compared the rate of regeneration of three equal pieces, each 10 mm. in length, from different levels of the same stem. I have not repeated these experiments as they seem to be lacking in point. The reason why the apical pieces in these experiments of Banus regenerate more slowly than the middle pieces is doubtless, as in the case of the other experiments reported in his paper, the consequence of an erroneous method of cutting the apical pieces. That some THE AXIAL GRADIENTS IN HYDROZOA. 393 basal pieces may precede the middle pieces is to be expected on the basis of what has already been said. It should be obvious that in stems as long as 30 mm., the length required for these experiments, the basal regions must already be more or less physiologically isolated as new individuals whether branches are present or not. Therefore it may be expected that at least some of these basal pieces will regenerate more rapidly than the middle pieces. It is furthermore to be remarked that the metabolic gradient is steepest near the hydranth and gradually diminishes in slope down the stem; and it has never been claimed by us that any marked axial difference exists along the basal parts of the stems of hydroids. Indeed, we believe that in many cases the gradient has disappeared in these basal regions, as shown by the tendency for such levels of the stem to produce numerous adventitious buds, irregularly arranged, while in the more distal levels of the stem, bud formation proceeds in a very definite and orderly manner. 4. The Effect of Depressing Agents. — It has been pointed out by us on numerous previous occasions that a certain relation exists between metabolic rate and depressing agents, such that regions of higher metabolic rate are more affected by depressing agents than regions of lower metabolic rate. If this general statement is correct, and various lines of evidence establish its accuracy, then it should be possible to reduce, eliminate, or reverse the differences in rate of regeneration that normally exist between apical and basal pieces. This is the case. Only two depressing agents were employed, ethyl ether and potassium cyanide. Apical and basal pieces of equal length were cut in the usual way and both exposed to the same concentration of these substances, made up in sea-water, for a certain length of time. The pieces were then thoroughly washed in several changes of sea-water and completed their regeneration in normal sea-water. These experiments are presented in Tables IX. and X. In Table IX. are given the results of all the mass experiments performed with cyanide. They were performed in June, at room temperature, except number 22, which was placed in the refrigerator later. The concentration of cyanide used and the number of hours during which the pieces were exposed to it are 394 LIBBIE H. HYMAN. given in the table. In experiment 7, the concentration employed, 1/20000 mol., was too weak to produce any effect, but the effects of i/ioooo and 1/5000 mol. solutions are very striking. The rate of regeneration of both apical and basal pieces is retarded, but that of the apical pieces, in accordance with the hypothesis, is more retarded so that the basal pieces regenerate on the whole the more rapidly. TABLE IX. RECORD OF MASS EXPERIMENTS ON THE RATE OF REGENERATION OF APICAL AND BASAL HALVES WHEN BOTH ARE EXPOSED FOR A NUMBER OF HOURS AFTER CUTTING TO THE SAME CONCENTRATION OF POTASSIUM CYANIDE. Columns give numbers of hydranths emerged at hours indicated. Exp. 10 control for exp. 13; exp. 21 for exp. 22. See Table III. Exp. 7. KNC 1/20000 Mol. for 20 Hrs. After Cutting. Exp. 13. KNC i/ioooo Mol. for 12 Hrs. After Cutting. Exp. 22. KNC 1/5000 Mol. for 9 Hrs. After Cutting. Hrs. a. b. Hrs. a. *• Hrs. a. b. 36 I 0 60 I o 84 i 0 40 2 0 62 2 2 93 2 O 46 5 2 *69 3 7 put into refrigerator 48 ii 3 72 8 9 50 13 8 74 12 14 178 2 I *58 18 16 76 13 16 205 7 3 61 22 18 78 15 20 2IO 8 7 64 23 20 80 17 20 216 8 9 67 21 83 20 23 *227 ii 13 73 23 85 24 24 229 ii 14 *93 34 36 231 12 16 IOI 35 40 237 13 20 106 35 41 *249 16 22 108 36 42 256 17 22 in 37 264 19 22 *I20 38 *273 23 23 276 24 23 280 24 26 283 24 29 287 26 29 *297 28 30 300 29 305 30 The results of the experiments with ether are given in Table X. Experiments 14 and 20 are mass experiments; 18 and 46 give records of the number of hours required for the regeneration of each piece. Experiments 14, 20, and 18 were performed in June at room temperatures, except that exp. 20 was kept in the refrigerator for part of the time after regeneration had begun. Experiment 46 was performed in December at a temperature of THE AXIAL GRADIENTS IN HYDROZOA. 395 12° C. ± 2. It will be observed that in the case of the mass experiments, the basal pieces on the whole regenerate more rapidly than the apical pieces, although both are retarded as compared with the controls. The individual experiments bring out the same point. The number of deaths was considerable but of 39 pairs in which both pieces regenerated, the basal pieces preceded the apical pieces in 14 cases, or 38 per cent, as compared with the result under normal conditions as given in Table V., where but 8 per cent, of the basal pieces precede the apical pieces. A number of interesting points are brought out by these experi- ments with ether and cyanide. In the first place the rate of regeneration is greatly retarded. In the case of cyanide, where different concentrations were employed, the retardation is pro- portional to the concentration used. This retardation is evi- denced by both kinds of pieces, the apical pieces being, however, more retarded than the basal pieces, with the consequence that the usual relation between the time of regeneration of apical and basal pieces is reversed. Now there can be little doubt that the rate of regeneration of pieces of Tubularia primarily depends upon the rate of chemical processes in those pieces. The effect of temperature upon the rate of regeneration is sufficient proof of this. Therefore, since depressing agents retard the rate of regeneration, it is impossible to doubt that they bring about this effect by lowering the rate of chemical reactions in the pieces. This is further evidenced by the fact that concentrations of these reagents which are effective at room temperatures are entirely without effect at temperatures of 12° and 13° C. Thus I per cent, ether is very effective at 20° C. but has no effect at 12° C. In order to alter the relations of apical and basal pieces at 12° C., it is necessary to use 2 per cent, ether. The same is true of cyanide. When, therefore, the metabolic rate is already lowered by low temperature, the action of depressing agents is diminished. This further supports the statement made at the beginning of this section that the action of depressing agents is related to the rate of chemical activity of the protoplasm which is exposed to them, and that such effects are greater the higher the metabolic rate of the living material. The differential effect, therefore, of ether and cyanide on the rate of regeneration of apical and basal 396 LIBBIE H. HYMAN. TABLE X. RATE OF REGENERATION OF APICAL AND BASAL HALVES WHEN BOTH ARE EXPOSED FOR A NUMBER OF HOURS AFTER CUTTING TO THE SAME CONCENTRATION OF ETHER. Experiments 14 and 20, mass experiments; experiments 18 and 46, individual experiments. Exp. 10, control for exp. 14; exp. 17 for 18 and 20; exp. 45 for 46; in Tables III. and V. Mass Experiments. Individual Experiments. Exp. 14, i <~-c Ether for 12 Hrs. After Cutting. Exp. 20. i <7 Ether for 12 Hrs. After Cutting. Exp. 18, i «7, Ether for 15 Hrs. Atter Cutting. Exp. 46, 2 <~( Ether for 17 Hrs. After Cutting. Hrs. a. b. Hrs. a. b. No. rt. b. No. a. b. 53 0 i 60 3 3 I 70 74 i dead dead 55 2 i 65 8 7 2 *87 US 2 dead *I37 59 5 4 67 8 12 3 *s- 90 3 dead dead 61 7 6 69 10 14 4 115 64f 4 dead dead *68 10 12 73 13 19 5 68 dead 5 9i 160 7i 10 14 75 U 19 6 74 94 6 IOO pit 73 14 16 *83 16 25 7 92 dead 7 94-5 95 75 16 19 88 17 8 87 US 8 137 I25t 77 20 23 90 19 9 99 dead 9 73 88 79 22 24 92 20 10 dead 99 10 89 79t 81 25 28 ii 99 96 f ii 113 127 83 26 29 12 74 7of 12 ; dead 125 *9I 30 32 13 dead dead 13 127 77t 94 31 34 14 dead 87 14 dead dead 99 33 35 15 87 dead 15 7i 9i 101 36 36 16 dead 76 16 100 7if 106 36 36 17 94 89t 17 95 *I37 *H5 37 36 18 96 dead 18 74 *89 129 37 37 19 90 *I07 19 *88 90 *i4O 37 38 20 63 63 20 68 dead 21 *88 112 22 90 IOO 23 *88 *88f 24 IOO 94t 25 *88 90 26 78 *n6 27 dead US . • 28 75 89 29 89 77t 30 dead 77 31 69 dead 32 77 9i 33 dead 77 34 69 *iii 35 *m 87t 36 91 101 37 89 98 38 *in Pit 39 7i 87 40 dead dead Total number of regenerated pairs , Number where a preceded Number where b preceded Number where a and b equal • 39 .24 or 61% . 14 or 38% i THE AXIAL GRADIENTS IN HYDROZOA. 397 pieces indicates very clearly that the apical pieces have a higher rate of chemical activity. They are more affected by the de- pressing agents and more retarded. 5. The Effect of Cutting the Distal End of the Apical Piece at the Base of the Hydranth. — When questioned regarding his manner of cutting the pieces for his experiments, Banus replied as follows (I quote verbatim from his letter): "The most distal cut was usually made as near as possible to the hydranth without includ- ing any part of it. Other times more basal parts were used. No difference in the results was found." To two subsequent letter* requesting more specific statements concerning this matter and asking for a diagram showing the exact relation of the most distal cut to the base of the hydranth, Banus returned no replies. The first sentence quoted leaves little doubt that Banus made his most distal cut just below the base of the hydranth, therefore including in the apical pieces, the little neck or stalk region of the hydranths. The rest of Banus's statement is too vague to merit any attention. What is meant by "more basal parts"? How is one to know in the experiments reported by Banus in which cases the distal cut was made at the base of the hydranth, and in which cases more basally? Certain it is that in some of Banus's pairs of pieces the apical piece emerges first, and in others the basal piece. This indicates some great irregularity in his method of procedure. Probably those cases where the apical pieces emerged first are the ones in which "more basal parts were used." In the absence of more definite information, specu- lation is idle. We are here concerned with the fact that usually the distal cut was made at the base of the hydranth. I have performed three experiments in which the apical pieces were cut in the manner usually employed by Banus and as represented in text-figure 5. Such apical pieces include the stalk of the hydranth. This stalk is incapable of regeneration. It together with that portion of the coenosarc which occupies the distal end of the perisarc dies away and disintegrates. This process of death and disintegration of the apical end of apical pieces cut in this manner naturally delays the regeneration of the apical pieces, because regeneration does not begin until the end of the piece has rounded off and become covered with a layer of LIBBIE H. HYMAN. cells. But this is not the only retarding factor in such pieces. The coenosarc after the death of the apical end withdraws into the perisarc leaving a short apical region of empty perisarc- This empty perisarc crumples to a greater or less extent. There- fore when the hydranth does regenerate it has to push out through this empty region before it can unfold, and this of itself would further delay the time of emergence of the oral hydranth; but to make matters worse, the crumpling of the empty perisarc renders it very difficult for the hydranth to push its way to the surface. On account of all of these factors, the regeneration of the apical pieces is very greatly delayed when they are cut in the manner employed by Banus. In fact, in many cases, the oral hydranth is so greatly retarded that the aboral hydranth emerges first, and in a few cases, the oral hydranth never emerged on such pieces, a complete reversal of polarity with disappearance of the primordium of the oral hydranth having been observed. Presumably Banus failed to notice whether oral or aboral hy- dranths had emerged, but the two ends of such pieces are easily distinguished by the bit of empty perisarc so that there is no doubt of the correctness of my statements. Not only are the oral hydranths of these pieces delayed but they are often ab- normal in appearance; they are enlarged and distended, owing probably to the pressure to which they are subjected in being forced out through the crumpled perisarc, and their tentacles are short and stumpy. They regulate to normal within a few hours after they have emerged. In two or three cases, partially doubled hydranths were produced. The three experiments performed with pieces cut in the way employed by Banus and as represented in Fig. 5 are presented in Table XI. They were performed in December and regenerated at a temperature of 12° C. ± 2. Experiment 31 consisted of pieces 5-8 mm. long, the other experiments of pieces 10-12 mm. long. In connection with experiments 39 and 49, the number of both oral and aboral hydranths emerged on the apical pieces at each observation is given. These records include of course only those cases in which the aboral hydranths emerged first. In some of these cases the oral hydranths subsequently emerged, and this is indicated by the number in parenthesis which follows THE AXIAL GRADIENTS IN HYDROZOA. 399 the number of pieces having aboral hydranths. In experiment 49, six such pieces had failed to give rise to oral hydranths when the experiment was concluded and in three of these cases, no primordia of the oral hydranths were present, the polarity having been completely reversed. The data given in Table XI. show in a very striking manner that the regeneration of apical pieces cut so that their distal ends TABLE XI. RECORD OF MASS EXPERIMENTS ON THE RATE OF REGENERATION OF APICAL AND BASAL PIECES OF EQUAL LENGTH WHEN THE DISTAL END OF THE APICAL PIECES is TAKEN AT THE BASE OF THE ORIGINAL HYDRANTHS. Exp. 31. Exp. 39. Exp. 49. Mrs. it. b. Hrs. a Oral. a Aboral. 6. Hrs. a Oral. a Aboral. 6. 60 i 2 62 5 o 6l 0 I *69 8 19 64 12 2 65 3 2 71 12 23 66 16 23 67 7 8 73 13 28 68 25 38 69 8 15 76 16 33 70 29 43 71 13 19 78 21 39 72 31 44 73 22 I 28 80 28 47 75 34 45 75 25 2d) 36 82 33 49 *82 42 47 *S5 31 4(l) 46 84 38 49 85 43 , 2 48 87 32 7d) 47 87 44 49 87 45 3 49 89 34 8(1) 49 *94 47 52 89 48 3 50 9i 35 10(2) 49 101 48 9i 48 3 5i 93 38 10(2) 49 103 49 95 49 3 Si 96 40 10(2) 49 105 50 *I07 50 3 52 98 40 10(2) 50 107 52 109 5i 3d) *IIO 44 10(4) in 52 3(3) are just at the base of the original hydranths is markedly delayed and that in the majority of cases, the basal pieces regenerate first. It should be remarked that the delay is chiefly in the time of emergence of the oral hydranths and not in its formation; for the primordia of the hydranths in these apical pieces form in advance as a rule of those of the corresponding basal pieces; but these hydranths can not emerge as rapidly owing to the fact that they must be pushed through the piece of empty crumpled perisarc left by the death of stalk region. The data in Table XI. furnish the explanation of Banus's results. It is obvious that anyone who practices the method of cutting the apical pieces described in connection with this table and who mixes up such a method with procedures where "more 4-OO LIBBIE H. HYMAN. basal parts are used" can expect nothing but irregular and inexplicable results. By using such methods and failing to de- scribe them it is possible to accumulate data which appear to contradict everything that previous workers have obtained. As' long as no one takes the trouble to inquire by what procedures such data were obtained and as long as the author refuses to furnish any information about his methods, such data might stand on record indenfiitely in the scientific journals to puzzle future investigators. I believe that I have conclusively shown that Banus's data are completely invalidated by his experimental method. This work and that of previous investigators — Driesch, Morgan, Child, Stevens, and Alice — demonstrate incontestably that in Tubularia when other factors are equal the rate of re- generation of pieces of equal size depends upon the level which they occupied in the intact stem; it is more rapid the nearer the pieces lie to the original distal end of the stem. A metabolic gradient exists in the stem of Tubularia which is the primary cause of these regional differences in rate of regeneration. H. SUMMARY. 1. This experimental work was undertaken as a reply to a paper published by Banus ('18). 2. The existence of a metabolic gradient in the stem of Tubu- laria is demonstrated in this paper in four different ways. (a) Differential susceptibility of apical and basal regions of the stem to ether and cyanide. Apical regions are more suscep- tible. (b) Differential capacity of apical and basal regions to reduce potassium permanganate. The apical end of the organism has the greatest reducing power. (c) Difference in electrical potential along the stem. Apical regions are electronegative (galvanometrically) to basal levels within the limits of the individual. (At a certain distance from the original hydranth of Tubularia a new individual is arising and the apical end of this is likewise electronegative to regions anterior to its level.) Since in general electronegativity is asso- ciated in protoplasm with increased oxidative metabolism, this difference in electrical potential along the stem of Tubularia is THE AXIAL GRADIENTS IN HYDROZOA. 40! evidence that distal levels have a higher metabolic rate than proximal levels. (d) Difference in the rate of regeneration of apical and basal pieces. Work upon this point constitutes the bulk of the paper and is summarized under the subsequent heads. 3. Apical halves of the stem of Tubularia regenerate oral hydranths markedly faster than basal halves. In cutting such pieces it is essential to discard the original hydranth and the first millimeter or two of the stem; the remaining stem is then cut into two equal halves. The difference between such halves has been demonstrated by: (a) Mass experiments in which all of the apical halves have been placed in one dish, the basal in another. In such cases, the number of apical pieces which have regenerated oral hy- dranths is nearly always in excess, rarely equal to, and never less than the number of basal pieces which have regenerated. (b) Individual experiments, in which the number of hours required for the emergence of the oral hydranth on each piece was recorded. The apical pieces regenerated oral hydranths first in 91 per cent, of the cases (122 pairs of pieces observed). 4. Apical pieces regenerate on the whole more rapidly than basal pieces, even when the latter are twice as long as the apical pieces. Such apical pieces must not however be less than 5 mm. in length. In pieces over 10 mm. in length, length has very little effect upon the time of regeneration; in pieces less than 10 mm. in length, the longer pieces regenerate faster than shorter ones having their distal ends at the same anterior level but this effect of length is not sufficient to overcome the influence of level except in very short pieces (under 5 mm.). 5. The difference in rate of regeneration of apical and basal pieces which exists under normal condition can be somewhat reduced by using stems bearing branches and cutting the apical piece above the branch and the basal piece below the branch. Since the first branch marks the limit of the Tubularia individual, the apical pieces above such branches are really the basal regions of the principal Tubularia individual, and the basal pieces below the branch are near the apical end of the second individual. In consequence of these relations, the difference between the 4O2 LIBBIE H. HYMAN. time of regeneration of such apical and basal pieces is less than is the case when pieces are cut from corresponding levels of stems without branches. 6. The difference in rate of regeneration of apical and basal pieces of the stem of Tubularia can be reversed by putting both sets of pieces for a certain time after cutting into appropriate concentrations of depressing agents like cyanide and ether. Under such circumstances the basal pieces regenerate in advance of the apical ones on the whole. This is due to the fact that depressing agents affect most strongly those regions having a higher rate of chemical activity. Since apical pieces have a higher metabolic rate than basal pieces, they are more affected by the same concentration of depressing agent and hence their regeneration is more retarded. In such cases the basal pieces regenerate the more rapidly. That this explanation is correct is further evidenced by the fact that the action of depressing agents is greatly influenced by temperature. At lowered tem- peratures a higher concentration of the agent must be employed to obtain the same effect produced at higher temperatures by lower concentrations. 7. These results are in accord with those obtained by a number of previous investigators and are directly opposed to the results presented by Banus. Banus claims that there is no difference on the average between the time of regeneration of oral hydranths on apical and basal pieces of the stem of Tubularia. Personal communication with Banus has elicited the fact that his usual method of cutting the apical pieces was erroneous. He cut them in such a way that the distal end of the apical piece was taken just below the base of the hydranth. In such cases, as shown in this paper, the distal ends of the apical pieces die away and the regeneration and time of emergence of the oral hydranth on these pieces is greatly delayed. It is believed that Banus's results are invalidated by such a method of procedure. 8. The results presented in this paper together with those of others quoted in the paper show that the rate of regeneration of pieces of Tubularia depends, when other factors are equal, upon the level which those pieces occupied in the intact stem; it is more rapid the nearer the pieces lie to the original distal end of THE AXIAL GRADIENTS IN HYDROZOA. 403 the stem. A metabolic gradient exists in the stem of Tubularie which is the primary cause of these regional differences in rata of regeneration. LITERATURE. Banus, M. Garcia. '18 Is the Theory of Axial Gradient in the Regeneration of Tubularia supported by facts? Jr. Exp. Zool., Vol. 26, pp. 265-275. Behre, Ellinor H. '18 An Experimental Study of Acclimation to Temperature in Planaria dorotocephala. BIOL. BULL., Vol. 35, pp. 277-317. Child, C. M. '07 An Analysis of Form Regulation in Tubularia. IV. Regional and Polar Differences in the Time of Hydranth-Formation as a Special Case of Regulation in a Complex System. Arch. f. Entw'lungsmech., Vol. 24, pp. 1-28. 'iga Demonstration of the Axial Gradient by Means of Potassium Perman- ganate. BIOL. BULL., Vol. 37, pp. 133-147. 'igb The Axial Gradients in Hydrozoa. II. Susceptibility in Relation of Physiological Axes, Regions of Colony, and Stages of Development of Certain Hydroids. BIOL. BULL., Vol. 37, pp. 101-125. Driesch, H. '99 Studien iiber das Regulationsvermogen der Organismen. II. Quantitative Regulationen bei der Reparation der Tubularia. Arch, f . Entw'lungsmech. , Vol. 9, pp. 103-136. Hyman, L. H. '18 Suggestions Regarding the Causes of Bioelectric Phenomena. Sci., N.S., Vol. 48, pp. 518-524. Loeb, J. '91 Untersuchungen zur Physiologischen Morphologic der Thiere. I. Ueber Hetermorphose. '92 Idem. II. Organbildung und Wachstum. Mathews, A. P. '03 Electrical Polarity in the Hydroids. Am. Jr. of Physiol., Vol. 8, pp. 294- 300. Moore, A. R. '10 The Temperature Coefficient for the Process of Regeneration in Tubularia crocea. Arch. f. Entw'lungsmech., Vol. 29, pp. 145-148. Morgan, T. H. '01 Regeneration in Tubularia. Arch. f. Entw'lungsmech., Vol. n, pp. 346- 381. '05 Polarity Considered as a Phenomenon of Gradation of Materials. Jr. Exp. Zool., Vol. 2, pp. 495-506. 'o6a The Physiology of Regeneration. Jr. Exp. Zool., Vol. 3, pp. 457-500. 'o6b Hydranth Formation and Polarity in Tubularia. Jr. Exp. Zool., Vol. 3, pp. 501-515. '08 Some Further Records Concerning the Physiology of Regeneration in Tubularia. BIOL. BULL., Vol. 14, pp. 134-149. Morgan, T. H., and Stevens, N. M. '04 Experiments on Polarity in Tubularia. Jr. Exp. Zool., Vol. i, pp. 559-587. THE GERM-CELLS OF CICADA (TIBICEN) SEPTEMDECIM (HOMOPTERA).1 ELMER L. SHAFFER, DEPARTMENT OF BIOLOGY, PRINCETON UNIVERSITY. CONTENTS. A. Introduction 405 B. Materials and Methods 406 C. Observations on Chromosomes AO7 1. Diploid chromosome groups 407 (a) Spermatogonia 407 (b) Follicle-cells 408 (c) Embryonic cells 410 (d) Somatic cells 410 2. Spermatocytes 411 (a) Growth stages 411 (b) Maturation tetrads 412 (c) Maturation Divisions 413 (d) Giant spermatocytes 414 3. Growth Stages and Synapsis in Oocyte 415 4. Chromatin Nucleoli in Oocyte 417 5. Discussion of Chromosomes 419 (a) Review 419 (b) Ring Tetrads 421 (c) Synapsis 422 D. Observations on Mitochondria 423 1. Mitochondria in Spermatogenesis 423 (a) Spermatogonia 423 (b) Spermatocytes 425 (c) Transformation of Spermatid 427 (d) Discussion 427 2. Nutrition of Egg 429 3. Mitochondria in Oogenesis 433 (a) Mitochondria in growth stages 433 (b) Yolk formation 435 (c) Discussion 438 4. Significance of Perinuclear Zone 441 (a) Relation to yolk-nuclei 441 (b) Origin of Mitochondria 443 E. General Considerations 446 1. Chromosomes 446 2. Mitochondria 448 F. Summary 450 1 A thesis presented to the Faculty of Princeton University, in candidacy for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy. 404 THE GERM-CELLS OF CICADA (TIBICEN) SEPTEMDECIM. 405 A. INTRODUCTION. The study presented here was done at Princeton University during the last year. The material was collected during the appearance of the ly-year locust (Cicada septemdecim) in the vicinity of Princeton, N. J., during the spring of 1919 at the suggestion of Professor E. G. Conklin. With such an interesting life cycle as these insects have, it was thought that a cytological study of their germ cells might reveal some important facts in the history of the mitochondria and the chromosomes. Both of these are constant and important structures of all cells, and it is the opinion of the writer that no cytological study can be complete which neglects either one or the other. It is only by a correlated study of cytoplasmic and nuclear structures that we can ever hope to solve the many perplexing questions in cell economy. We use the almost mystic phrase: "interaction between nucleus and cytoplasm" in many cases to cover our ignorance concerning certain cell activities, but we are far from knowing any of the specific actions and reactions between the nucleus and cytoplasm. WTithin recent years the study of mito- chondria of animal and plant cells has attracted many workers, with the result that many cytologists have come to regard these structures as of vital importance in cell activities. Attention, which for many years has been centered on the nuclear activities, has been drawn to a more intensive study of the cytoplasm and its structures. All these studies have emphasized the importance of such structures as mitochondria in relation to cell metabolism. Moreover, although the chromosome hypothesis of heredity seems to be firmly supported by a vast amount of evidence, yet one group of cytologists (Meves, Benda, Duesberg, etc.) maintain that the mitochondria also have a role as the bearers of hereditary characters. While the evidence which these workers have gathered is not of a convincing nature, nevertheless the facts are worthy of careful consideration. There are some reasons for believing that inheritance in some cases is through the cytoplasm, and we must not loose sight of the fact that "cytoplasm as well as nucleus is concerned in heredity and differentiation" (Conklin, '16). However, whether or not such cytoplasmic structures as 406 ELMER L. SHAFFER. mitochondria constitute the idioplasm is an entirely different problem. Modern researches in genetics have shown that, despite mutations, hereditary characters are relatively stable and that the hereditary constitutions of organisms are definitely organized; hence the idioplasm which is causal in the develop- ment of the hereditary characters must similarly be stable and highly organized. Keeping in mind the "cell as a whole," I have studied the chromosomes and mitochondria in the oogenesis and spermato- genesis of Cicada, and my observations give added evidence to the chromosomes as the idioplasmic substance, while there is no evidence from an unbiased standpoint that the mitochondria behave as idioplasmic substances. There is evidence, however, that the mitochondria are intimately concerned in cell meta- bolism. Throughout this work I have had the constant advice and encouragement of Prof. E. G. Conklin, and it is with great pleasure that I here express my indebtedness. B. MATERIALS AND METHODS. The youngest specimens of Cicada obtained were those of the second pupal stage about three weeks prior to their emergence from the ground and their final moult into the imago. These specimens were collected about the middle of April by digging under trees in the vicinity of Princeton. The pupae were found lying about a foot from the surface of the ground and were most abundant several feet away from the tree trunks. In the testes of such pupae, one finds most of the cells in the maturation stages besides an abundance of spermatids, spermatozoa, and a few spermatogonia. In the adult or imago, the testes are almost completely filled with sperm except for a small number of spermatogonia. After copulation, the testes are reduced to about one tenth their former size and contain only a small residue of spermatozoa, some degenerated cells and a few spermatogonia which also show signs of degeneration. There are two testes, each consisting of a great many radiating ellipsoidal follicles which give the testes a berry-like appearance. In the female there are two typical ovaries, each consisting of a THE GERM-CELLS OF CICADA (TIBICEN) SEPTEMDECIM. 407 great number of ovarian tubules containing a great many oocytes. The oldest oocyte of a second stage pupa is about one seventieth the linear size of the oldest oocyte of the adult, which shows the tremendous growth that takes place in a few weeks. It has been estimated (Marlatt, 1898) that the female Cicada lays between 400 and 600 eggs. The male and female gonads were dissected out in Ringer's solution and fixed in either Flemming's (strong), Bouin's, Benda's, or Regaud's fixing fluids. Mitochondria were well preserved by the Flemming, Benda and Regaud fluids, but were either partially or wholly destroyed in Bouin's fluid depending on the length of time the gonads remained in the fixing fluid. Material fixed in Flemming's fluid (10 to 12 hours) was usually the best for studying both chromosomes and mitochondria. Sections were cut 8 to 10 micra in thickness and the stains used were iron-haematoxylin (with and without counterstain), Benda's crystal-violet and alizarin, and Altmann's fuchsin-methylene green. The developing eggs were also collected from time to time for a study of the chromosomes of the embryonic cells. These were fixed in either Bouin's or Carnoy's fluids and were imbedded by the celloidin-paraffine method. C. OBSERVATIONS. i . Diploid Chromosome Groups. (a) Spermatogonia. — The spermatogonia are found in the proximal end of the ellipsoidal follicles of the testes. They form a cap of cells at this end of the follicle containing the primary and secondary spermatogonia. From the proximal end of the follicle there proceeds a short narrow filament which contains the spermatogonia of the multiplication stages. Mitotic figures are quite abundant among the spermatogonia of the multiplica- tion stages, but the metaphase plates are usually so crowded that it is impossible to make accurate counts of the chromosomes. On the other hand, the primary and secondary spermatogonia rarely show cell divisions, but the metaphase plates when they appear are very clear. The male chromosome number is 19, which indicates that there 408 ELMER L. SHAFFER. is present an unpaired sex element (Figs. I and 2). One pair of the complex is strikingly larger than the rest, being in the form of somewhat curved rods (Text-fig, i). This pair corresponds to the " macrochromosome " pair described by Kornhauser ('14) in Enchenopa. Two other chromosome pairs (BB, CC, Text-fig, i) can also be distinguished from the other chromosomes by their size, being approximately half the size of the macro-chromosomes (A A). The other 13 chromosomes show no size differences which would enable us to arrange them in pairs or distinguish TEXT-FIG, i. Spermatogonial chromosomes, showing the relative sizes of the chromosomes; the macrochromosome pair, A A, the BB and CC pairs, and 13 other chromosomes which show no size differences. them from each other. However, as will be later shown, the AA, BB, and CC chromosome pairs are so characteristic in their form and size that they can be recognized in all the diploid groups. The size relations of the chromosomes of Cicada corre- spond to those described by Kornhauser ('14) in Enchenopa. Also in the Cercopidce (Homoptera), Boring ('13) has described three pairs of chromosomes (A, B, C) which bear similar size relations to the three pairs here described in Cicada. The sex-chromosome cannot be identified in the spermatogonial groups either by its size nor by any peculiarities in its staining reactions. In the resting spermatogonia there is, however, always present a single chromatin nucleolus (Fig. 13) which probably is the persisting sex chromosome. (b) Ovarian Follicle Cells. — Among the follicle-cells of the ovary, mitotic figures are very abundant. I have found a great many mitoses not only among the follicle-cells surrounding the young growing oocytes, but even among the follicle-cells sur- rounding the almost mature oocytes. I have searched for evi- dences of amitotic division among these cells, which has often THE GERM-CELLS OF CICADA (TIBICEN) SEPTEMDECIM. 409 been described, especially in the follicles surrounding mature oocytes; but I have failed to find any strong evidence for the occurrence of this method of nuclear division. The only indica- tion that amitotic division may take place is found in the follicles surrounding the old oocytes. Here the cells are usually bi- nucleate and appear to be the end stages of amitotic divisions. However, I have found many karyokinetic figures among such cells and it seems reasonable to suppose that the binucleate follicle-cells arise through the failure of the division of the cell body following mitotic division of the nucleus. All metaphase plates of dividing follicle-cells show 20 chromo- somes (Figs. 3, 4, 5, 6, Text-fig. 2), and it is possible in all of these to recognize the chromosome pairs AA, BB, and CC as in the spermatogonial plates, by their size relations. Figure 12 is that of the late telophase of a dividing follicle cell in which c 9 /ET* «i«:.* TEXT-FIG. 2. Two metaphase plates from ovarian follicle-cells showing the A A, BB, CC pairs of chromosomes; note that the A A pair lies in the center of the group. one of the macrochromosomes (A) can be recognized in the daughter cells. It will also be seen that there is present a precocious longitudinal split of the chromosomes in preparation for the next cell-division. In the follicle-cells surrounding the older oocytes, the chromo- somes do not have the thick compact appearance found in the younger follicle-cells. The chromosomes- are usually thinner, poor in chromatin content, and the respective chromosomes appear somewhat longer, often showing an equational split (Figs. 10, n). I am unable to account for this difference in appearance of the chromosomes (compare Figs. 7, 10, n). In 4-IO ELMER L. SHAFFER. the resting nuclei of the old follicle-cells, it is very noticeable that they are very poor in chromatin content, all the basichromatin being accumulated in one or two small masses (nucleoli). When such cells prepare for mitosis, the chromosomes which are re- constituted are correspondingly poor in chromatin. However, the linin basis of the chromosomes is still present and conse- quently the number and size relations of the chromosomes is maintained. This, I believe, gives added evidence to the view previously expressed (Shaffer, '20) that the linin is the morpho- logically stable substance which maintains the chromosomal organization and structure. (r) Embryonic Cells. — Although a number of the developing eggs of Cicada were collected, I was unable to obtain good material for a study of the chromosomes due to the difficulties in sectioning. The eggs were so full of yolk that it was possible to cut them only by imbedding by the celloidin-paraffine method. Figure 9 is that of a metaphase plate of a cell from the blasto- derm showing 20 chromosomes, and hence of the female type. The chromosome pairs AA, BB, and CC can be distinguished as in the other diploid nuclei. While I have been unable to make an exhaustive study of the chromosomes of the embryonic cells, yet I have found no variations in the chromosome numbers or in their size relations. (d) Somatic Cells. — On dissecting the female locusts to remove the ovaries, a number of round, brown-pigmented bodies re- sembling eggs were found in the abdomen. On sectioning these it was found that they were of a glandular nature and are perhaps concerned in the secretion of adhesive materials for the eggs. In a small cap of cells which lies at one end of these glandular bodies, mitotic figures were found in abundance. There are alwrays 20 chromosomes which show similar size relations to the diploid chromosome groups previously described. After mitotic division of the nucleus the cell-body fails to divide, resulting in the formation of binucleate and multinucleate cells. At the time of division of such multinucleate cells, typical tri- asters are formed (Fig. 52). In the telophase of such a division, the tripartite daughter nuclei reconstruct to form six separate nuclei, the cell-body again failing to divide. At this time the THE GERM-CELLS OF CICADA (TIBICEN) SEPTEMDECIM. 4! I cytoplasm becomes active in the secretion of large globules and although the nuclei increase in number, I have been unable to find mitotic figures and it seems possible that the increase in their numbers is brought about by amitosis. 2. Spermatocytes . (a) Growth Stages. — Unfortunately the material wh'ch was collected showed very few of the early growth stages of the spermatocytes and consequently I was unable to make a detailed study of the process of synapsis. Apparently the early growth stages must take place some time before the month of April. In the youngest pupae which I have collected, the only growth stages of the spermatocytes which I have been able to find are those of the pachytene-bouquet stage (Fig. 17) in which the thick synaptic threads are polarized at one side of the nucleus. Sections across the bouquet usually show 18 chromatic blocks representing the end view of the threads. Since each loop has been cut twice, this would indicate that there are 9 pachytene loops. At the base of the polarized bouquet is usually found the compact, deeply-staining nucleolus (Fig. 17, A") which is the persisting sex-chromosome. This is undoubtedly the same com- pact chromatic nucleolus found in the resting stages of the spermatogonia (Fig. 13). As is usual, the bouquet is polarized toward the pole of the cell containing the idiozome (Fig. 17, id.). It has often been stated that the idiozome exerts some attraction on the synaptic threads influencing their polarization. There is no evidence to support this view and I am inclined to believe that the same factors which determine at which point in the cell the idiozome should lie also determines the polarization of the synaptic threads. Occasionally the pacyhtene threads show a longitudinal split, and in such cases it is noticed that the chromatic granules (chromomeres) of the two halves of the thread do not correspond either in size or location. Consequently the longitudinal split cannot be interpreted as an equational split, but is rather the primary split or point of synaptic union. Usually there is present a single loop of the bouquet which is much larger than the other loops (Fig. 17, A A) and this undoubtedly represents 412 ELMER L. SHAFFER. the synaptic condition of the macrochromosome pair of the spermatogonial chromosomes. (6) Tetrads and Maturation Divisions. — Stages of the early prophases of the spermatocytes were quite abundant and it was possible to follow the formation of the first maturation tetrads. In the early prophases, the homologous threads show a great variety of twisting about each other (Fig. 20). One pair (A A, Fig. 20) is easily distinguishable from the others by its large size and is derived from the spermatogonial macro- chromosomes. Figure 18 represents the stages in the formation of the definitive tetrad from this pair (Figs. 61, 62, 63, 64). In the early prophases the homologous threads of the AA tetrad are very long and twisted about each other. However, they retain their connections at the ends, thus making the tetrad a large ring if its twists were straightened out. The space en- closed by this ring is the interchromosomal space which marked the point of synaptic union of the threads. In the later prophase stages, the large ring condenses, the threads become thicker retain- ing their point of union at the ends and the interchromosomal space becomes reduced in size until in the definitive maturation tetrad it is reduced to a small oval slit between the two halves of the ring (Figs. 18, 21, 62). In the first maturation metaphase, the macrochromosome tetrad no longer appears in the form of a ring, but rather in the form of a ring flattened at the poles, or as two slightly bent rods whose concavities oppose each other. In a similar way, the tetrad derived from the BB pair of the spermatogonia goes through the formation of a ring tetrad (Fig. 19), resulting in a tetrad similar to the macrochromosome tetrad (A A), but approximately half its size. The other tetrads show no ring formation; usually the homologous threads become free at one of the synaptic ends, retaining their connection at the other end, thus giving the appearance of two chromosomes joined end-to-end (Fig. 20). The condensation of such tetrads produces the typical dumb-bell form tetrad, with the narrow portion of the dumb-bell marking the retained point of synaptic union. Thus, if the point of synaptic union is retained at both ends, rings like the A A and BB tetrads are produced; if the synaptic union is retained at one end only, the dumb-bell type THE GERM-CELLS OF CICADA (XIBICEN) SEPTEMDECIM. 413 tetrad is produced. Payne ('14) has described a different method of ring-formation in Forficula. The two univalent chromosomes are first joined end-to-end; while retaining this point of union, the free ends come together by a bending process with the result- ing formation of a ring each half of which represents a univalent chromosome. A similar method of ring-formation has been described by Sutton ('02) in Brachystola and by Davis ('08) in several Orthopterans. In Forficula besides the "bending pro- cess" of ring-formation, Payne describes ring-formation of the type here described in Cicada. (r) Maturation Divisions. — In the metaphase plates of the first maturation division, the chromosomes are always grouped in a characteristic manner (Figs. 23, 24, 53 to 57). There are 10 chromosomes in the metaphase plate, 8 of which are arranged in a circle surrounding the macrochromosome tetrad (A A , Fig. 23) which always lies in the center of the group. The sex chromosome (Fig. 23, X) always lies outside this circle of chromosomes and often does not lie in the same plane. Boring ('07) has figured the chromosomes of a number of species of Homopterans in which the sex-chromosome lies outside the group of autosome tetrads. The constant position of the AA tetrad in the center of the spermatocyte complex can be traced back to the diploid chromo- some groups in which the macrochromosome pair shows a marked tendency to lie in the middle of the metaphase plate with the other chromosomes grouped about them (text-fig. 2). In a previous paper (Shaffer, '20) it was pointed out that the char- acteristic grouping of the chromosomes in the metaphase had its explanation in the persistence of the interchromosomal linin fibres (Fig. 24) which undoubtedly persist as a part of the chro- mosomal architecture and maintain definite spatial relations between the chromosomes. The evidence in Cicada seems to support this view. In the side-view of the metaphase, the AA and BB tetrads are arranged on the spindle in the direction of the spindle axis. Hence in polar views only a half of each tetrad can be seen (Fig. 23). In both the A A and BB tetrads the spindle fiber attach- ments are median (Figs. 18, 19) or atelomitic (Carothers, '14), and since the tetrads lie in the direction of the spindle axis, they 414 ELMER L. SHAFFER. will separate in the anaphase at the point of synaptic union and the division is reductional. The other tetrads have terminal spindle fiber attachments, but I am unable to say whether they divide reductionally or equationally. In the case of the CC tetrad, there is evidence that the narrow portion of the dumb-bell actually marks the point of synaptic union, and since separation of the dyads in the anaphase occur at this point, the division is also reductional. The sex-chromosome (Fig. 25, X} usually lies on the outer surface of the spindle. In the anaphase it usually lags behind the other dividing chromosomes, sometimes appearing bipartite, and passes undivided to one of the daughter cells (Figs. 27, 70). As the dyads come into the late anaphase of the division, a secondary (equational) split can often be seen (Fig. 27, A A}. Following the first maturation division, there is no inter- kinesis or construction of a telophase nucleus. The dyads again become arranged in the metaphase, each showing the secondary split. Figure 28 (also Fig. 58) is that of second spermatocytes (daughter plates), one having 9 dyads the other having 9 dayds plus the X-chromosome. It will be noted that the grouping of the dyads is exactly similar to the grouping of the tetrads in the first maturation division metaphase, namely 8 dyads arranged in a circle around the macrochromosome dyad. In Notonecta, Browne ('16) found that the chromosomes always assumed a definite grouping in the metaphase, but the grouping was different in the two maturation divisions. In the anaphase of the second maturation division all the dyads divide and there are no lagging chromosomes. (d) Giant Spermatocytes. — It is quite common to find spermato- cytes with double or more the number of chromosomes. Figure 59 is a photograph of such a spermatocyte in the metaphase which has over twice the normal number of tetrads. These giant spermatocytes develop normally and give rise to giant spermatids and spermatozoa (Figs. 31, 33 •» ^Tvl .. / v * -'>> .*c^-v / Q 8 t5 o 36 39 ' i^m^j - ELMER L SHAFFER ELMER L. SHAFFER. PLATE VI. (Fics. 44 TO 48). FIG. 44. Portion of nucleus (nd.) and cytoplasm (cy.) of oocyte showing migra- tion of mitochondria from perinuclear region toward the periphery of the cell. X 1,200. FIG. 45. Ingestion of nurse-cells by upper end of egg-string (e.s.). Products of ingestion are seen passing down egg-string into cytoplasm of oocyte. Note perinuclear arrangement of mitochondria. FIG. 46. Germinal vesicle of old oocyte showing plasmosome (pi.) and several chromatic nucleoli. X 800. FIG. 47. Portion of oocyte and its follicle. Mitochondria arranged in periphery of cytoplasm. Persistence of egg-string. X 600. FIG. 48. Stages in the transformation of mitochondria into yolk-sperules. BIOLOGICAL BULLETIN VO . X>XNI. i! '£ *i ^|p£? '•"'"-•JCS'-J"' . ^^k ' ./":?; V *.w ^'. ** r • £ ^ J i. /i. c. .<"*•»- " -t^ •• I '. tv^; i'"- ^t5- * XA\ 198? t>L li^ff $*$& %&v»* "TI.C, "-ooct. • -e. s. O '• © V » . - V 0 J. c. /.- • « .o •- '• j A -V •" * v • flSsKpry •• v >.«»'ST;. -- ^ • » • • ' • » .,. ^ ELMER L. SHAFFER. 47O ELMER L. SHAFFER. PLATES VII, VIII, IX. Photomicrographs taken at a magnification of about 1,000 diameters, except Nos. 71 and 72 which have been magnified 500. The reproductions here have not been reduced. PLATE VII. (Fics. 49 TO 70). FIGS. 49, 50, 51. Metaphase plates of follicle-cells of ovary, showing 20 chromo- somes among which the macrochromosome pair can be distinguished. Fig. 51 is at a greater magnification. FIG. 52. Tripolar spindle in one of the cells of the adhesive gland in the female. FIGS. 53, 54, 55, 56. Metaphase plates of first spermatocytes from four different animals, each showing 10 bivalent chromosomes which are similarly grouped in each case. FIG. 57. An oblique section through the metaphase plate of the ist maturation division, showing the two halves of the macrochromosome in the center of the complex. FIG. 58. Daughter plates cf second spermatocytes, one with 9 dyads, the other with 9 dyads plus the sex-chromosome. Note that the grouping of the chromo- somes is the same as in the first spermatocyte. FIG. 59. Giant spermatocyte with a great many bivalent chromosomes and a large amount of mitochondria. FIG. 60. Anaphase of first maturation division showing mitochondria sur- rounding spindle. FIGS. 61, 62, 63. Various forms of ring tetrad (macrochromosome tetrad) in the early prophases. FIG. 64. Macrochromosome tetrad in late prophase. FIG. 65. Late prophase of first spermatocyte, showing character of tetrads. FIG. 66. Anaphase of first maturation division from smear preparation, showing separation of macrochromosome dyads. FIG. 67. Ring tetrad in prophase of first spermatocyte; from smear prepara- tion. FIG. 68. First maturation division showing separation of macrochromosome dyads. FIG. 69. Anaphase of first maturation division showing separation of the dumb-bell shaped tetrads. FIG. 70. Late anaphase of first maturation division, showing lagging sex- chromosome. BIOLOGICAL BU.LETIN, VOL. XXXVIII. 4 9 50 52 53 » *. ;*• 55 **» 57 •> 64 65 66 • £* /V 6/ I •*• ••* V 70 L. SHAFFER 472 ELMER L. SHAFFER. PLATE VIII. (Fics. 71 TO Si). FIGS. 71, 72. Portion? of the nurse chambers of the ovaries, showing fibrous appearance of central plasmatic mass in which the egg-strings of the oocytes end. Nurse-cells in various stages of ingestion may be seen and the products of their disintegration may be seen passing down the egg-strings into the oocytes. The oocytes are distinguishable by their deeply staining perinuclear zone of mito- chondria. Figs. 73 to 75 are various stages of synapsis in oocyte. FIG. 73. Protobroque nucleus at beginning of synaptic period. Two chromatic nucleoli are present. FIG. 74. Deutobroque nucleus of oocyte. Treads become more evident; two chromatic nucleoli are present. FIG. 75. Leptotene stage. Polarization of the threads. Chromatic nucleoli are absent. FIG. 76. Pachytene bouquet stage. FIG. 77. Section across bouquet stage, showing 20 threads on end view. FIG. 78. Strepsistene stage. Reappearance of the two chromatin nucleoli. FIG. 80. Oocyte from ovaries fixed in Bourn's fluid (10 hours) showing effect of acetic acid on mitochondria. Vacuoles, globules and nucleolar-like structures are found in cytoplasm due to the partial dissolution and agglutination of the mitochondria. FIG. 81. Oocyte after fixation in Benin's fluid for six hours, showing perinuclear zone of mitochondria only partially destroyed. Two chromatin nucleoli are present in the nucleus. BIOLOGICAL BULLETIN, VOL. XXXVIII. I . v;~v i 7-4 * 76 ELMER I . SHAFFER 474 ELMER L. SHAFFER. PLATE IX. (Fics. 82 TO 87). FIG. 82. Oocyte with its egg-string, from ovaries fixed in Bouin's fluid 15 hours showing the complete disappearance of mitochondria. FIGS. 83, 84. Oocytes from Flemming fixed material showing characteristic arrangement of mitochondria in a perinuclear zone sharply delimited from the rest of the cytoplasm. FIG. 85. Oocyte at the period when the cytoplasmic volume has reached its maximum. The mitochondria still surround the nucleus, but the zone is not so sharply delimited. Shrinkage of the nucleus away from the cytoplasm shows the nuclear membrane to be intact. FIG. 86. Older oocyte in which perinuclear arrangement of mitochondria is lost, the granules becoming scattered toward the periphery of the cytoplasm. FIG. 87. Typical germinal vesicle of almost mature oocyte, showing two chromatic nucleoli. '-' J,. « , , i + 86 • ! ElMER L. SHAFFER MBL WHOI LIBRARY IdH 17KA 1 \