ttlOLOGY LIBRARY G 1butcbfn0cm'0 mature Xttrarg BIRD BEHAVIOUR OTHER VOLUMES OF Hutchinson's Nature Library ALREADY ISSUED INSECT ARTIZANS AND THEIR WORK By EDWARD STEP, F.L.S., author of " Messmates," " Toadstools and Mushrooms of the Countryside," etc. MESSMATES ; A Book of Strange Companionships By EDWARD STEP, F.L.S., author of "The Ro- mance of Wild Flowers," "Shell Life," etc. With 55 illustrations from photographs on art paper. THE COURTSHIP OF ANIMALS 2nd Edition. By W. P. PYCRAFT, A.L.S., F.Z.S., Zoological Department British Museum, author of " A History of Birds," " Story of Reptile Life," etc. With numerous illustrations on art paper. BY THE SAME AUTHOR THE INFANCY OF ANIMALS 2ndEdmon With 64 plates on art paper and numerous illustra- tions in the text. MM* YOUNG OF BARN-OWL. Although their white down is very different from the feathers of the old birds these nestlings have the face just like the adult. TYPES OF PHEASANTS. Argus (top left), Golden (top right), Mikado (centre), Silver (bottom), showing different styles of masculine decoration. Frontispiece. BIRD BEHAVIOUR PSYCHICAL AND PHYSIOLOGICAL BY FRANK FINN, B.A. (OXON), F.Z.S. Late Editor of " The Zoologist" AUTHOR OF "BIRDS OF THE COUNTRYSIDE," "EGGS AND NESTS OF BRITISH BIRDS," "THE WORLD'S BIRDS," ETC. " Behold the fowls of the air. "-ST. MATTHEW vi. 26 WITH 44 ILLUSTRATIONS ON ART PAPER LONDON HUTCHINSON AND CO. PATERNOSTER ROW BIOLOG LIBRARY TO THE ANIMAL-DEALERS OF LONDON IN ACKNOWLEDGEMENT OF THEIR SERVICES TO SCIENCE * • « • • • "•* I ''"I: D.ECJICATE THIS BOOK CONTENTS CHAPTER I Importance of subject — Incompleteness of our information about even the commonest species — Distinction between major and minor habits — General activities or tricks of manner — Greater importance of the latter in classification — Errors to be avoided in observation . . . . pp. I- 1 2 CHAPTER II The locomotion of birds — Hopping and walking — Reasons for adoption of these gaits — Why waterfowl waddle — Swimming and diving — Perching and climbing — Different methods of performing these actions — Specialized birds which have taken to different habits, as Ground-Parrots and Land Geese — Flight and its varieties — Characteristic methods according to group and size — Sailing and soaring flight — Speed . pp. 13-32 CHAPTER III The nutrition of birds — Various kinds of food, animal and vege- table— Methods of and adaptations for obtaining it — Changes of diet — Gluttony of some species — Power of discrimination among foods, both vegetable and animal — The much-discussed relations of birds to insects, especially butterflies pp. 33~7^ 3748 vi CONTENTS CHAPTER IV Nutrition (continued) — Manipulation of food — Powers of diges- tion, differing in different groups — The formation of pellets or castings — Difference in the food of old and young in some cases — Different methods of feeding the young — Young assist- ing parents in feeding their juniors — Feeding of each other by the sexes — Drinking, and eating of such substances as salt and earth ..... . pp. 77-112 CHAPTER V Propagation — Care of young — Different types of young birds — Different modes of feather-development, as seen in young Fowl, Pigeon, or Duck, for instance — Egg-coloration and its meaning and variations — Prolificacy and otherwise — Incuba- tion mounds — Periods of incubation . . pp. 113—165 CHAPTER VI Propagation (continued) — Nest-making not purely a bird-habit — Eggs laid without nests — Types of nests — Parasitic nesting — — Parasitic layers, like Cuckoos and Cow-birds — Degrees of development of parasitic instinct . . . pp. 166-205 CHAPTER VII Migration — An anciently observed phenomenon still imperfectly understood — Reasons for it — Methods as far as is known — Difference between migratory species and the homing Pigeon — Widespread tendency toj migration, contrasted with con- tradictory tendency to form localized non-migratory races, ending in some cases in Sightlessness, as in some birds of remote islands . . . . . . pp. 206-224 CONTENTS vii CHAPTER VIII The senses of birds — Sight and its general high development — Degree of perception of colour — Influence of colour, if any, on courtship, and the segregation of species — Perception of the colour in various kinds of food — Smell, usually poorly developed — Exceptions noted — Acuteness of hearing — Sense of touch — Taste-perceptions . . . pp. 225-254 CHAPTER IX The emotions of birds — Mentality higher than is supposed, but variable according to species or groups — Strong- and weak- minded birds — Intelligence and stupidity — The limitations of instinct — Expression of the emotions and its relation to courting displays — Love and sociability — Hatred and re- venge— The police instinct — Monogamy, polygamy, and polyandry — The problem of preferential mating pp. 255-280 CHAPTER X Song and cries of birds — Bird-language generally — Extent to which the notes are instinctively developed — The instinct of mimicry — Species which can imitate human speech — Problem of this ability and extent of exercise of the same — Possibility of understanding of bird-language by man . pp. 281-292 CHAPTER XI Weapons and fighting methods of birds — Their combats with each other and with various natural enemies — Chief enemies of birds — The passive resistance of birds to unfavourable climate and surroundings — Natural defences — Perfection and degeneracy of plumage in this connection — Powder-coating of some groups pp. 293-302 viii CONTENTS CHAPTER XII Special instincts of birds — The play of young birds and of adults — Bower-builders and their peculiarities — Ornamentation of nests — The instinct for food-storage in some forms — The practice of piracy — Toilet and bed-time habits pp. 303-318 CHAPTER XIII Special physiological peculiarities of birds — Longevity — Tempera- ture of body — Change of colour in bare skin of some, such as Turkey — The phenomena of the moult — Gradual change in colour of bill and feet according to age and sex or season — Changes in iris colour — Beak-sheath shedding as in Puffin pp. 319-328 CHAPTER XIV - Abnormalities — Hybrids, their characteristics and power of repro- duction or otherwise — Abnormal plumages, such as albinism or melanism, temporary or permanent — Overgrowth of claws and bill pp. 329-342 CHAPTER XV Relations of birds with men — Persecuted species — Parasitic or commensal species — Domestic forms — Introduced forms and the results of introduction . . . .pp. 343~3S6 INDEX pp. 357-363 ILLUSTRATIONS YOUNG OF BARN-OWL . . . Back of Frontispiece TYPES OF PHEASANTS .... Frontispiece FACING PAGE THE BIRD-COLONY IN THE CALCUTTA Zoo . . 10 NEST OF SOCIABLE WEAVER-BIRDS . . .11 PUFFIN ...... 36 GREAT BLACK WOODPECKER .... 36 MERGANSER ....... 3^ SHOVELLER ........ 3^ FLAMINGOES ....... 37 AVOCET ........ 46 HUIA (FEMALE) . . ... . . .46 HUIA (MALE) 46 HERON ........ 46 RHINOCEROS HORNBILL ..... 47 SPOONBILL ........ 47 PEREGRINE FALCON ...... 47 FLAMINGO ........ 47 RHINOCEROS HORNBILLS ..... 50 Toco TOUCANS . . . . . . • 51 ix x ILLUSTRATIONS FACING PAGE LYRE-BIRDS (MALE AND FEMALE) ... 54 IMPERIAL WOODPECKERS . . . -55 BOURU FRIAR-BIRD ...... 82 BOURU ORIOLE, SHOWING RESEMBLANCE TO FRIAR- BIRD .83 YOUNG HOATZIN . . . . . • *32 BRUSH-TURKEY 133 OVEN-BIRD .133 CONCAVE-CASQUED HORNBILL . . . .170 BRAZILIAN HANG-NEST I?1 TYPES OF PENSILE NESTS 180 NEST OF CENTRAL-AMERICAN SWIFT . . .181 GuACHAROS AND NEST . . . • .184 FLAMINGO ON ITS NEST . . . . .185 SwiFTLETS AND THEIR NESTS . . . .185 HAWK-CUCKOO OR " BRAIN-FEVER BIRD " . . 194 SHIKRA HAWK . . . . • • • *95 AMERICAN KING-BIRD . . . . . 278 KING-CROW 279 AFRICAN JA9ANA . . . . . • 296 CUBAN TROGON 297 GARDENER BOWER-BIRD WITH ITS BOWER . . 310 NEWTON'S BOWER-BIRD 311 HYBRID BETWEEN GOLDEN PHEASANT AND FOWL . 330 HERRING GULLS 33 l BIRD BEHAVIOUR CHAPTER I Importance of subject — Incompleteness of our information about even the commonest species — Distinction between major and minor habits — General activities or tricks of manner — Greater importance of the latter in classification — Errors to be avoided in observation. THE study of birds is often looked down upon by general zoologists as a trifling pursuit, and the reason of this is not far to seek ; what is called zoology nowadays is for the most part the study of com- parative anatomy, and from this point of view birds are of extremely small interest ; they are remarkably uniform in their general structure, and such varia- tions of note as do occur are chiefly confined to a few flightless families, such as Ostriches and Pen- guins. One need not, indeed, be an anatomist to realize the comparatively small structural interest of birds ; a bird is at once and by everybody recognized as such, while among the mammals, their rivals in high development, one gets such extraordinarily different types as whales, mice, bats, horses, lions, i 2 ':• •': BIRD BEHAVIOUR adftd meti,' most of which differ from each other more than trie earliest known bird, the Arckao'pteryx of Jurassic times, does from the modern Sparrow in the back-garden. On the other hand, the numerous species and families of birds and their close alliance with each other afford, by this very narrow range of differentia- tion, an attractive and philosophical study ; among them survive types which in mammals have become " missing links " or are only discoverable in the fossil state. In fact, it is the survival of so many connecting forms that makes it so difficult to group the families of birds into larger " orders," a difficulty which only occurs with mammals when we look back among their fossil predecessors preserved among the rocks. We look, for instance, with interest upon the remains of the various ancestral predecessors of the horse family, as exhibited in museums, and try to realize what changes of habit must have occurred to convert a small animal with paws into a large one with a single hoofed toe on each foot. But among the birds, if we go to the Duck family (Anatida) we find still in existence practically all the links between a light-bodied, large-winged bird with non-waterproof plumage and half-webbed feet with large grasping hind-toe, and almost exclusively aquatic diving Ducks which rival the Grebes and Cormorants in their subaqueous per- formances, and exhibit almost as much modification of structure. And the habits of these can be DUCKS AND EVOLUTION 3 studied, for they are not only living, but accessible ; the semi-wader at the beginning of the series, the Australian Magpie-Goose (Anseranas stmipalmata) is no rarity, and I have spent much time in study- ing a full-winged specimen, at Kew, while the habits of many others of the family exemplifying the gradations I have mentioned are accessible either to direct observation or to any one who can look up bird literature. In fact, the Duck family is one of the most inter- esting of all animal groups for any one interested in biological problems, owing to the wide distribu- tion of its members, their essential and obvious alliance combined with equally striking differences — some obviously adaptive, others more inexplicable — and I shall have a good deal to say about them in the course of this book, especially as they are familiar exhibits in many public parks, and thus available for everybody's observation. The availability of birds, as a class, for study gives them, in fact, an importance in the study of animal habits which fairly outweighs their insignificance from the morphological point of view ; and I personally have never admitted that the study of structure is more important than that of habit, considering that it only acquires its pre-eminence from the fact of demanding a professional training which is the prerogative of a few only. When one comes to think of it, we ourselves only surpass the other mammals in virtue of our habits, being structurally simply monkeys on our hind legs ; yet 4 BIRD BEHAVIOUR he would be a bold zoologist who would claim that comparative anatomy is a more important study than that of the manifold activities of mankind. If one admits that the study of habit is to be taken seriously — and of late there has been a decided trend in that direction — bird-watching needs no apology, for the habits of birds are of manifold variety, and not by any means fully understood, even in the case of the commonest and most familiar species. In fact, these are sometimes, perhaps, less well understood than the comparatively inaccessible ones ; it has often struck me, in view of the excellent observations that have been made of late years in the Antarctic by various expeditions, that we know more about the mind and life of the Adelie Penguin, one of the remotest birds on the globe, than we do about those of the Peacock, the best known by sight and reputation of all birds for a couple of thousand years. And here one must bear in mind that an observer should be no respecter of persons ornithologically ; a bird is not necessarily more worth observing because it is difficult of access, and that naturalist was very unscientific who said about the Sparrow, " I have got into the habit of not noticing this bird." Per- sonally I am always seeing something fresh in the humble Sparrow's performances, and though my taste as a fancier lies in the direction of birds of beauty, I must admit that the humbler species are often more interesting. But they are not necessarily so ; dowdiness is no more a sign of FREE AND CAPTIVE BIRDS 5 intellect in a bird than in a human being, and Aristotle was quite right in stigmatizing some birds as " dull in colour and leading a dull life," while some of those which are stigmatized as " garish " possess habits and qualities of surpassing interest, the Peacock in particular. I particularly mention the Peacock, because it is a purely Indian bird, but widely diffused about the world in a domestic state ; but its domestication is not rigorous, so to speak, and it is allowed to lead practically a natural life. This needs to be noticed, because there is a regrettable tendency among naturalists to confine their observations to the wild birds native to their own country, to the neglect of introduced, domes- ticated, and captive species. A free bird is, of course, other things equal, the best and most instructive subject for observation, and many habits can only be observed on birds in a state of liberty, and in their own country at that ; but nevertheless, many very interesting hints may be gleaned from the study of birds not so situated, and these may always be checked by the study of their recorded habits in a natural state. Where such records do not exist, the study of captive birds is a useful stimulus to field observers to take up the matter, and often an observation on a tame or captive bird brings out a point which the field observer almost necessarily overlooks, owing, in many cases, to want of opportunity to discriminate between individuals — a point which will become evident later on. 6 BIRD BEHAVIOUR The best of all birds for observation are, of course, those which live in a tame but free state in their own country, such as Wood-Pigeons in London, and House-Crows (Corvus splendens) in Indian towns, though even here the difficulty of identifying individuals is a drawback. The growing practice of ringing birds, however, which is proving so useful in throwing light on problems of migration, will probably be of service in making records of individual birds in the near future, and in this connection it is worth noting that spring net-traps can be obtained in which many species of birds can be readily captured without injury for this purpose. In considering the habits of birds we have to realize that they fall into two categories, which may perhaps be called major and minor habits, or per- haps, habits and customs. By major habits I mean the leading and conspicuous life- activities of a species ; its food, manner of nesting, mode of association (solitary or gregarious), etc. On these its life obviously depends, and to them its structure is often plainly adapted. Yet, as its life depends on the adjustment of its main activities to its sur- roundings, it is just these habits that are particularly liable to vary even in a state of nature, and especially so under the influence of man's interference, direct or otherwise. Habits of this sort are fairly, though not completely known in the case of a vast number of birds, but will always require study, as they are so subject to modification by circumstances. The minor habits, or mannerisms as we should VERSATILITY OF SPARROW 7 call them in the ease of persons, are not nearly so familiar, and have not been jvorked out fully even in many very well-known birds. Such are pecu- liarities in gait, flight, and other actions ; attitudes under emotion, positions assumed in repose, etc. Often these have no apparent connection with any necessity, and they are practically invariable for the species and often for the group, and not alter- able by circumstances. To take a concrete instance ; the common Spar- row, as every one knows, eats seed, for the cracking of which its bill is specially adapted ; it perches in trees, and has feet adapted for grasping twigs ; it also builds its nest in trees, and associates in pairs and flocks. These are its major habits, and every one knows they are subject to modification ; it eats many things besides seed, especially remnants of man's food ; it builds under eaves as well as among boughs, and will sleep in its nest or in a crevice of a wall, as well as on a twig, in spite of its grasping feet. In spite of its short wings, it chases insects in the air, and hovers and drops on them in long grass like a miniature Kestrel. In other words, its major activities are, though to some extent correlated with its structure, highly variable, and this, no doubt, is one great reason for its success as a species. If actively interfered with by man it can vary still more ; a hand-reared Sparrow has been known to acquire a Linnet's or Canary's song. In some points, however, the Sparrow is invari- 8 BIRD BEHAVIOUR able ; it never acquires the habit of holding down its food with one foot, though this would be of vital importance to it in enabling it to consume large articles of food when perched on a small bough, and thus escape risk of ground enemies. Though largely a ground-feeder, it hops and does not run, and does not hold up its tail to keep it clear of wet ; when courting it has a definite display, with wings lowered and head and tail raised. It rolls in dust as well as washing, a com- bination of habits rare among birds, and when scratching itself it lowers its wing and raises its foot over it. Some of these minor habits may have significance, but this is not obvious ; what is obvious is that they are common to all Sparrows, and are often group-characters ; thus, all the vast group of generally small birds — the Passerines, of which the Sparrow (Passer) is type and name-father — seem to drop a wing when scratching. Habits of this kind do not alter even in captive birds, although they may learn strange habits of feeding and nesting, and even alien songs. Hence such characters are often useful in classi- ficatipn, just as anatomical characters or inconspicu- ous external ones are of more importance than the general contour. Thus, all anatomists of late years have impressed upon us that the Swallows and Swifts must not be considered near relatives because of their similar forms — long-winged, small- footed, and short-billed — since these may be ac- counted for by adaptation to a similar life spent in SWIFTS AND SWALLOWS 9 chasing flying insects, and the anatomical characters of the Swifts differ from those of the Swallows, which present no important internal differences from the ordinary small passerine birds^ There are small differences also in external characters which are not to be correlated with modes of living ; the Swifts have but ten tail-feathers to the Swal- lows' twelve, and have not the scales on their shanks which are usual in birds. There is also a conspicuous difference in habit which seems to have no relation to utility, and hence is more likely to be a family character ; in flight the Swallow every now and then draws in its wings to its sides — it still shows a sign of the typical small passerine bird's flight with its occasional dips with closed wings — while the Swift always keeps its wings fully expanded, whether skimming motionless or renewing its impetus by means of wing-beats. Small details of habit are thus always worth observing, as they may be more significant than they seem, and in any case are good practice in observation. Some pitfalls in observation or in drawing con- clusions from the same need mentioning in con- clusion. The danger of the major habits becoming modified in captivity or domestication has been alluded to, and is, indeed, made rather too much of by some writers ; still, it is well to bear it in mind. There is also the difficulty of the field observer in discriminating between individuals — often between the sexes, when these are alike. One must never io BIRD BEHAVIOUR assume anything ; if a bird is displaying, it is not necessarily a male, for instance. Quite apart from the fact that in many species the hen is the dominant sex, as in Phalaropes and Cassowaries, the hen may display even in those in which the male is well differentiated and very self-assertive ; thus, I have seen a Peahen display to a Peacock, and a hen Turkey to the gobbler, these males in both cases remaining passive, so that had the sexes been alike, one would certainly have been tempted to draw very false conclusions about their behaviour. Then one must be very cautious in keeping free from prepossessions ; it is quite easy to see what is not there, if one has some image already in one's mind. We laugh at the error of the old naturalists who credited the Osprey, as a fishing- bird-of-prey, with one taloned foot and one webbed one ; these odd extremities no doubt seemed to them appropriate, but I have seen almost equally gross instances of faulty observation of points of form and colour in the work of modern naturalists of the best repute. Such errors can, of course, easily be checked, but when it comes to actions, unless the camera is at hand for the recording of such (which can be but rarely the case), the evidence is necessarily dependent on a number of witnesses. Every one is liable to err, and any observer is liable to have the opportunity of observing an action or occurrence which is rare or strange, and will be disbelieved until some one else arises to coirfirm it. H rt H ai 3 I W M o « NEST OF SOCIABLE WEAVER-BIRDS. " Republican Grosbeak " was the old name for this Weaver, which is a plain-plumaged, Sparrow-like bird. INCONSISTENT CORMORANTS 11 One should thus be very careful in discrediting the observations of others if they do not happen to agree with one's own, especially if such were recorded many years ago ; a record is not necessarily bad because it is ancient, nor good because it is new. Even one's own observations may contradict each other at times, as happened to me in my experience of our wild colony of the small Indian Cormorant (Phalacrocorax javanicus) in the Calcutta Zoo. These birds came in the evenings to their roosting-place on a wooded island in a large pond, and usually swooped down, Swallow-fashion, to take a drink on the wing before going up to roost. This was an unusual feat for a Cormorant to per- form (although it must be remembered that this species is a light-built, long-tailed bird of only the size of a Teal), and, from the way in which the birds began to gape before touching the water, and often involuntarily checked their way so much that they had to settle after all, they evidently found it a difficult one. It so happened, too, that once for some time they gave the habit up, and settled in flocks in the water to drink in the normal way, though they afterwards resumed the custom of the flying sip ; and I can well imagine that any one who had seen them thus drink sitting would put down my record of the flying drink as most far-fetched and out of all congruity with the structure and habits of a Cormorant. Very likely the birds had adopted a really new habit, in drinking flying ; at any rate, we know 12 BIRD BEHAVIOUR new habits do occur — I rather fancy that I dropped across one of late, in finding the Sparrows peeling off the inner bark of the limes for nesting- material, even when they could easily get straw, and I find the habit not uncommon in the Regent's Park district, as many barked branches testify. There is, therefore, always room for observation, confirmatory or original, and no conscientious observer need fear to record what he or she sees, for there is no such thing as authority in science, and the veriest beginner may often put the expert to the blush. CHAPTER II The locomotion of birds — Hopping and walking — Reasons for adoption of these gaits — Why Waterfowl waddle — Swimming and diving — Perching and climbing — Different methods of performing these actions — Specialized birds which have taken to different habits, as Ground-Parrots and Land-Geese — Flight and its varieties — Characteristic methods according to group and size — Sailing and soaring flight — Speed. So old an author as Pliny gives some remarks on the locomotion of birds, many of them quite accurate, as when he points out that Crows walk and Spar- rows and Blackbirds hop. Such differences in action are well known to most people, but it is just as well to have them summarized. The usual gait of birds is a walk, that is to say, when they are considered by groups ; it is true the majority of small birds one sees are hoppers, but that is because the common small birds of most parts of the world are passerines, and in this group hopping is the usual gait, walking being customary only in some of the larger species, such as Crows, and in groups which, like Crows, habitually seek food or even live on the ground, such as Wagtails, 13 14 BIRD BEHAVIOUR Pipits, Larks, and Starlings. As passerine birds are all primarily adapted for a life in trees, even the Larks still having a typical percher's foot, and as jumping from bough to bough is a natural and habitual movement, it seems natural that this should be continued in all except those most thoroughly adapted to a ground life. The same reasoning applies to the Hornbills, among which family are to be found the only large birds which hop ; there are two species of this usually very short-legged and highly arboreal family which have legs of the ordinary length and live mostly on the ground, the Ground-Hornbills (Bu- corax), and these move their legs alternately in the ordinary walking fashion and are even able to run well, which some walking birds do not do — most Herons and Storks, for instance. In the case of these Hornbills, it is to be noted that the hind toe is just as well developed as in the perching kinds, and that they walk on the end joints of the toes, the ball of the foot being raised above the ground, so that one could put a marble under it. This would be unique among birds, which usually tread on the whole under-surf ace of the toes, where these are on the same level — i.e. the hind toe not set on higher — were it not for the case of the Ostrich, which lifts the basal joints of its two toes off the ground even more, so that its foot can fairly be said to have a pastern-joint, like that of the hoofed beasts with which it habitually grazes. In the Ostrich reduction of the toes is carried to an HOPPERS AND WALKERS 15 extreme, but in most running birds the hind toe is absent or greatly reduced, and useless, so that the case of the Ground-Hornbills, Larks, Desert- Choughs (Podoces), and other running members of perching groups is unique, as they alone of all running creatures have the bearing-surface of the foot well developed behind as well as in front. Unfortunately for our explanation, the Hornbills and passerines do not exhaust the list of groups of birds in which some members run and others hop ; and in the other cases it is difficult to assign a reason for the difference of gait. Pigeons, except some (not all) of the most terrestrial kinds, are all short- legged, and most live largely in trees, but they nearly all walk, although some of the most arboreal Fruit-Pigeons do hop as well ; I have seen this most in the Lilac-crowned Fruit-Pigeon (Ptilopus coronulatus). Parrots, again, are mostly tree-birds, and all short-legged, some very much so, but they generally walk, although the Lories form an excep- tion by hopping. Kingfishers do not move on their feet at all if they can help it, and all without exception are very short on the leg ; yet the great Australian Laughing Jackass, which is more of a ground-feeder than any other, is a hopper, while the few others I have been able to study all walk, or rather waddle. So do Bee-eaters, at least the Australian and European species (Merops ornatus and M . apiaster) ; but of two Indian kinds of Nightjars I have seen one walk and the other hop, and the only Trogon (Prionotelus Umnurus) whose 16 BIRD BEHAVIOUR gait I have ever studied, hopped; and all these birds are short-legged. The shortest-legged Passerines, too, are the Swallows, but these walk when on the ground instead of hopping. Among Cuckoos and Rollers, the practice is more rational, so to speak, for the short-legged species hop, while the longer- legged Ground- Rollers and the magpie-like Bush- Cuckoos, like the Indian Crow-Pheasant (Centropus sinensis), run. Toucans, Barbets, and Woodpeckers all seem to hop, and this about exhausts the list of hopping birds, all of which, it will be noticed, are essentially perchers, so that hopping undoubtedly has something to do with the perching habit. Walking-birds, however, may hop when in a hurry, as one may see with Rooks, Starlings, and even Wood-Pigeons, while birds of prey, which normally walk, though but little, hop so readily when " cornered" by a broken wing as to have given rise to the notion that hopping is their natural gait. Conversely hopping birds of the Thrush tribe often take a run for a few feet. Waterfowl have a bad name for clumsiness in walking, and certainly some of the more specialized kinds waddle awkwardly on account of the wide distance between their legs, which is convenient for swimming, but necessarily produces a rolling gait. This also badly handicaps them in perching, which so many of them do, especi- ally in hot climates, for they find it difficult to walk along a small bough ; and in the case of the perching Ducks, the dwarfed hind- toe is of no use for a grip, so much so that I have seen such active species as PERCHERS AND CLIMBERS 17 the Mandarin Duck and Andaman Teal (Ncttium albigulare) slide right over and off a perch on which they had tried to alight. Only Cormorants and Darters among waterfowl seem at all at home in trees, and these have a powerful gripping foot with well-developed hind-toe, and more freedom in the legs than is usual in diving birds. In travelling along a perch, the most active, as well as the clumsiest perchers, are apt to move sideways ; thus the lively Sparrow and the heavy Cormorant both sidle on a bough, though the ground-living Fowl and the arboreal Parrot walk foot over foot, for which the in-turned feet of the latter are particularly suitable. The most active birds at this pole-walking are the Guans and the Touracous, which run along boughs like squirrels, and at the same time can leap long distances from one perch to another; but on the whole, the more active the bird, the more apt it is to adopt the sidelong gait on a bough, often turning round with each hop, a method of procedure which seems calculated to make it giddy. In climbing, several methods are employed ; Parrots, as every one has seen, hook themselves along with their beaks as well as their claws, though they do not do this in the wild state as much as one would think from seeing them caged ; and Cross- bills, those Parrot-like Finches, climb in the same way. Woodpeckers and typical Creepers, however, climb quite differently from Parrots, but like each other, though the last group are Passerines and not 2 1 8 BIRD BEHAVIOUR related to the former ; both hop upwards or side- ways on boughs and trunks, resting on their stiff- pointed tails, and letting themselves slip back if, which is seldom the case, they want to descend. The Nuthatch, which is not stiff-tailed, has the advantage of these extreme specialists in being able to climb in any direction, down as well as up. Swimming is generally performed by alternate strokes of the feet when the bird is on the sur- face, and simultaneous strokes when it dives ; but Auks, though they swim like other birds on the surface, propel themselves when below with their half-closed wings, and the Penguins never use their feet in the water at all, apparently, but rely entirely, both on and under the surface, on their flipper- like wings. Land- birds can gener- ally swim if they are put to it, those which run swimming with their legs — I have seen a young Peacock thus save himself ; those which rely on their wings will flap themselves ashore, which I have seen a Swallow do after it had fallen in ; but they soon become draggled and exhausted, and except for the large flightless runners, cannot go any distance, in spite of being unable to sink. The giant runners, however, are as strong swimmers as beasts. I have known of a Cassowary making land after a four miles' swim in a by no means calm sea. Land-birds when swimming sink low in the water, but, curiously enough, so do some of the most specialized divers ; the Cormorants swim with the tail awash, and the allied Darters only show the LAND GEESE AND GROUND-PARROTS 19 head and long neck above water, most amply justifying the name of Snake-bird given to the Indian species. It is curious to note how one finds some swimming birds completely deserting the water, and taking to a land life, though still retaining nearly complete webs to the feet; for instance, the Australian Cereopsis Goose (Cereopsis novte-bollandia) seems never to go into water except to wash, or to escape when wounded, and the Hawaiian Goose (Nesocben sandvicensis), which lives on the old lava-flows in the mountains of its native islands, exists, during most of the year at any rate, without any water at all, even for drinking, getting the needful moisture from its food of berries and succulent herbs like sow-thistle. Conversely, we have, in the Dippers or Water-Ousels, Thrushes with ordinary perching feet which swim and dive. So also one may find the Parrots, so peculiarly adapted for a life in trees, keeping not only to ground life like Partridges in the case of the Aus- tralian Ground-Parrakeet (Pezoporus formosus) and Night-Parrakeet (Geopsittacus occidentalis), but even waxing fat and losing the power of flight, in the case of the New Zealand Kakapo or Owl-Parrot (Stringops babroptilus), which looks like a gigantic degenerate form of the last. And among Cuckoos, birds also specialized mostly for tree-life, we have the swiftest runner of all birds in the American Road- runner or Chaparral-cock (Geococcyx mexicanus), which is only about the size of a Magpie, and yet 20 BIRD BEHAVIOUR can run for hundreds of yards ahead of a horse or dog, being thus proportionately swifter than the Ostrich. It is curious, by the way, that no hopping bird attains high speed or power of long-distance travelling on foot, although, judging from what Kangaroos and Jerboas can do in this way, one certainly would have expected this to happen. In the flight of birds there is of course less possible variation than in their movements on land and trees, or in water, but still there are some very characteristic differences. What may be called the normal or usual flight is by continuous strokes, as is .usual in Pigeons, and universal in the Duck tribe ; but this progression may be interrupted either by sailing intervals, both by slow-flapping birds like the Gannet, or quick-whirring ones like the Partridge; or by closing the wings and so dropping, to rise again with a fresh flutter, which is the usual method in Passerine birds, Wood- peckers, and Barbets, all tree-birds or relatives of such, be it noted, and most of them small, so that the method is probably adopted to gain impetus : just as many birds, such as Ducks and Parrots, roll much in their flight, apparently to put a " screw " on to themselves and increase pace. In the Hoopoe the flap and closure of the wings alternate so quickly that the bird looks just like a big butterfly. The resemblance of many Parrots to wildfowl in their flight is indeed curious ; both groups stretch out their necks in front and their feet behind, although this backward extension of the feet was SOAKERS AND SAILERS 21 supposed to be a speciality of the long-legged waders, the extended limbs acting as a rudder. But as a matter of fact the extension of the legs behind in flight is normal among birds generally, the only groups that draw up their legs to the breast being Passerines, Woodpeckers, Barbets, and Hoopoes, all undulating flyers. The extension of the neck is also normal in many groups, but Passerines, Hoopoes, Rollers, birds of prey, Herons, Pelicans, Frigate-Birds, Petrels, Gulls, Shore-Birds, and the Carrion-Storks (Leptoptilus) draw it in, so that here there is more latitude allowed, so to speak. The finest flyers of all seem generally to favour the drawn-in neck, at any rate that is the pose with Eagles, Vultures, and Alba- trosses, whose powers of maintaining flight for long periods together without movement of the wings have always evoked admiration, and still remain unexplained. Albatrosses have narrow though very long wings, and usually fly low ; the overland soarers have shorter but very broad wings, and only sail when flying high. Both groups, however, are noticeably large birds ; small birds of any groups, no matter how well-winged, never soar or sail very far. This may be due to the need for weight to give steadiness, but it is at least quite as likely that it is connected with the more lively disposition of small species, and with their habit and need of picking up their food in small bits at frequent intervals, so that they never have either the time or inclination for soaring. 22 BIRD BEHAVIOUR In respect of the activities of the soaring birds of prey, it has been noted that the highest soarers are the latest to rise, the Vultures not taking the sky till after the Kites, and none soaring till the sun is well up, and it has been suggested that sun-lit air has some favourable influence on soaring. Even over here I have observed that a fine, sunny day in winter or early spring will set the Black-headed Gulls on the Thames soaring, but I always put this down to their being inspired by the fine weather with thoughts of their home breeding-grounds, and a preparation for travelling thither ; while I should have said that the connection between sun and soaring in the East was simply that the larger carrion-feeders needed clearer air, and had to mount higher, as their food was harder to find ; but after an absence of a dozen years from India I am quite willing to admit I may be mistaken. However, sunny weather in autumn does not seem to take the Gulls aloft, as I have had an oppor- tunity of observing recently. It is noticeable that soaring birds are never still, but keep on describing circles as they float on extended motionless wings ; as a matter of fact they appear to descend a little and then use the impetus thus gained to rise again, so that they are really making spirals. A bird, except in a strong wind, can only stand still in the air by constant and rapid wing- action ; this is the feat of hovering, of which the Kestrel is the most familiar exponent in this country during most of the year, though the FLIGHT OF HUMMING-BIRDS 23 Sparrow frequently practises the trick over long grass in summer in order to locate insect prey. The Kingfisher also frequently hovers, when a handy perch to swoop from is not available, and in India the Pied Kingfisher is the most familiar of hovering birds, this species seeming always to make its survey of the fishing-ground on the wing. Hoverers of the more skilled types raise and lower themselves with great ease and skill, such flying reaching its perfection in the Humming-birds, which almost invariably hover when feeding, and dart sideways or even backwards as unconcernedly as dragon- flies. They seem indeed to represent these insects in the bird world, for like them they are no pedestrians, only using their feet for perching or clinging, since if any bird is unable to progress on the ground it would appear to be these, and they seldom shift their position even when perched. In conformity with their insect-like flight, the wings of the Humming-birds are much like those of insects in shape, the segments of the arm between the pinion and the body being much reduced, so that there is little action except from the shoulder. Not all of them, however, move their wings with the insect-like quickness which in the small kinds results in the hum that gives these truly fairy creatures their name, for the largest species (Pata- gona gigas), which is as big as a Swift, flaps its wings at an ordinary rate of speed and quite visibly. The slowing of the wing-stroke in accordance with increase of size seems to be a general rule in birds. 24 BIRD BEHAVIOUR always supposing t^ose of the same family are compared ; thus the common Grey Heron flaps in a very leisurely way, while the little Dwarf Bitterns (Ardetta) use a quick fluttering stroke like a Moorhen, and the slow heavy sweeps of the Goose contrast forcibly with the quick wing-beats of the Duck. Comparing a Duck and a Heron of about the same size, however, the Duck will be found to move its wings more quickly, the Ducks being a quick- action family as a whole, and the Herons a slow one. This makes the comparatively quick action of the small Herons interesting, as the wings in all this family are large for the weight of the very light body, so that we must not suppose that quick wing-action is always connected with small wing- area, though this is often the case, as in such birds as Auks, where the wing has been reduced, to serve also as an oar, to the minimum size consistent with flight. The Great Auk went further — and fared worse, for when man came upon the scene some time in the Stone Age, it began to discover the disadvantages of turning its pinions completely into paddles. The great soaring and sailing birds also find they " cannot have it both ways " ; they are adepts at saving their petrol, so to speak, and this is a point which aviators would do well to study, as it would be of great advantage if they could circle by the hour without using their engines ; but they are bad and heavy starters, rising at a low angle and DEATH-TRAPS FOR PIRATES 25 with great effort. This is particularly the case with the Albatrosses, which have sacrificed muscular power to gliding capacity, and elongated and nar- rowed their wings, to a very risky extent. On the Australian coast there is a valley ending in a cliff-wall which is a regular death-trap for the local Albatrosses which pass over it on their way home to a breeding-ground. If, when coasting over this hollow, they dip between the walls, they lose the wind, begin to drop, and not having suffi- cient strength of wing-beat to "get up steam " in the limited space in front of them, end by colliding with the cliff at the end of the gully, when they fall to the ground to die a lingering death from hunger, for the walls of the valley are too steep for them to climb, and they have not enough intelligence and enterprise to explore a cave at the cliff-foot which would lead them out into the open again. Another example of the dangers of over- develop- ment of wings is to be found in the appropriate fate of hanging that now and then befalls the piratical Frigate-bird in the West Indies, as described by Dr. P. Lowe ; this species, like so many tropical waterfowl, frequents trees, but may easily miss its footing when alighting and slip down among the twigs, where its great wings cannot have full play, and it is exceedingly likely to catch its neck in a fork, when its very much reduced feet are of little use in its attempts to extricate itself, and it soon perishes. 26 BIRD BEHAVIOUR The Frigate-bird is, however, a better starter than the Albatross, being able to " take off " at once. The easiest of all starters are the Gulls, for they can spring up from a sitting position if they like, without troubling to rise to their feet. When Swifts start off the ground, they seem to " take off " with their wings, like Bats when simi- larly situated ; the idea that they cannot thus rise is certainly erroneous so far as the common House-Swift of India (Cypselus affinis) is concerned, as I have proved by repeated experiments. With regard to the power of springing up vertically at the start, the Game-birds are unrivalled, on account of their muscular power both in legs and wings. The best settlers seem to be the Hawks ; it is delightful to observe one of these birds just glide up to a perch and touch it at exactly the right moment, no final flapping being required ; such accuracy of action being no doubt a necessary accomplishment in birds which have to grasp food when in flight and avoid injuring themselves by concussion in so doing. But such skill has to be acquired with practice j I have seen a young Kite in India plunge up to its belly in a pond when picking up an object which an old one would have skimmed off with hardly a ripple. As a contrast to the skill of the Hawks, we may take the awkwardness of the Grebes, which trail their legs even when alighting and strike the water anyhow. I am sorry I never noted what, if any, difference there is between young and old Kites in the matter QUADRUPEDAL LOCOMOTION 27 of soaring ability ; but Buller, in his " Birds of New Zealand," mentions that the immature speci- mens of the very common Harrier of that country (Circus gouldi), which are more conspicuously differ- ent in colour from the adults than are the young Indian Kites, do not soar as the old ones do, so that they are often taken for a different species of Hawk altogether* In very rare instances young birds may even fly better than adults ; this is the case, according to Mr. W. H. Hudson, with the common Tinamous of Argentina ; and in that very curious bird the great grey Steamer-Duck of the coasts of southern South America (MicropUrus cinereus)^ only the younger individuals seem to be able to fly at all, the older birds becoming so heavy that their small wings simply serve to support and assist them when running on the surface of the water, a curious quadrupedal mode of locomotion which has attracted the attention of all observers, but seems only to have been fully and exactly described by Mr. M. Nicoll in his admirable work on his experiences as a naturalist in the cruises of the Earl of Crawford's yacht the Valhalla. So fast can these huge Ducks " steam " along, he tells us, that a six-oared boat cannot overtake them or even come within shot. Quadrupedal locomotion on land is found in the case of the Penguins, which when pressed will use their flipper-wings as fore-legs, the species which inhabit snowy regions tobogganing along most effectually in this way. As we shall see later, also, 28 BIRD BEHAVIOUR some young birds, even among those of our own country, employ quadrupedal locomotion. With regard to the speed of birds in flight, some very " tall " statements, especially of German manufacture, have been circulated ; for there is a strong tendency to exaggeration about this point. Before steam-engines came in, the speed of birds' flight could not be so well estimated as it can now ; as Newton justly remarks in his famous " Dictionary of Birds," the Swallow does not usually travel so fast as an ordinary express train ; and yet this bird is proverbial for swiftness. The speed of the common Pigeon is also well known owing to the popularity of Pigeon-races, and these show that it is a good Pigeon that can do its fifty miles an hour ; while the most ordinary observation shows that the Pigeon, though not nearly equal in swiftness to the Swallow, is yet much faster than the majority of birds. The speed of small birds tends to be exaggerated, owing no doubt to the quickness with which they move their wings ; thus, Common Teal are credited with being the fastest of Ducks by some writers, but this can hardly be the case, for in India the Shoveller, and even the heavy Spotted-billed Duck (Anas fcecilorhyncha) — the Mallard of India — have been recorded as leading a bunch of Teal ; and in America a flock of the very closely allied Green- winged Teal (Nettium carolinense) have been seen so hard pressed by the apparently slow-flying White-headed Eagle that they had in desperation RECORD PACE-MAKERS 29 to drop into the water headlong, a common man- oeuvre with Ducks when a pursuer's speed is too much for them. The slow-flapping flight of large birds as com- pared with their more briskly moving relatives in the same family has long been recognized by sports- men as very deceptive ; wild Swans, Capercailzie, and wild Peafowl are all birds which need the shot to be aimed well forward if they are to be fatally hit, their flight being so very much faster than it looks, especially to a shooter who has been used to smaller fry. The fastest birds of all appear to be, as one would expect, some of the Swifts, the palm among these being assigned by Blanford, in the " Fauna of British India," to the larger species of Spine-tails (Chatura), which are stated by observers to whizz by with a twang like a bowstring. It is noticeable that the spiny-tipped tail in these birds, which, like the similarly armed tail of most Woodpeckers and Creepers, serves as a support when the owner is clinging perpendicularly, is so short that it cannot be of much use in directing their flight, and in fact the Swifts generally, though better flyers than Swallows, distinctly tend to be shorter in the tail, and often dispense with the fork. In fact, the opposite development of the tail in birds which fly well is very curious, some fine flyers having a long forked tail, like Swallows,, Terns, and Frigate-birds, while others, like the above- mentioned Swifts, most Albatrosses and Vultures, 30 BIRD BEHAVIOUR and that aerial acrobat the Bateleur Eagle (Helo- tarsus ecaudatus) seem to have gone in for tail- reduction, so that the steerage, etc., is evidently given over entirely to the wings in their case. The steering action of the tail is particularly- observable in Kites, in which that member is often turned almost into a vertical plane, and this charac- teristic action was noted by the ancient Romans, Pliny suggesting that it was the Kite that taught men the use of a rudder to a ship. Generally speaking, however, the most important use of the tail is as a brake when descending ; why it is always expanded at starting is not so obvious, and probably the action is an involuntary one, like the dropping of the legs by Gulls and Crows when they check in their flight, a habit which has led some observers into the mistaken idea that Crows, when picking food off water, do so with their feet, which is never the case so far as I have observed. Mere speed of flight, by the way, is not neces- sarily so indispensable an asset for escaping aerial enemies as may be supposed ; Swallows and Swifts certainly suffer little from birds of prey, the Hobby being the only Falcon which ordinarily catches them, and the swift Sand-Grouse, among ground- birds, are too much for most Falcons. But some quite slow birds are also very difficult subjects for Hawks, owing to the facility with which they shift from the stroke, the Hoopoe and Lapwing being notorious for their abilities in this respect ; TERNS AND TROPIC-BIRDS 31 while as to the slow-flying Grass-Owl of the East (Strix Candida), a near relative of the Barn-Owl, Indian falconers say it is one of the birds which 5 1 O a in o S^ W -* II AN ADVANCED DUCK 47 called "Paradise Duck" in that country; in this the plumage is affected, the drake being a dark grey with a greenish-black head, while the duck is mostly chestnut- red with a snow-white one, and is thus far more striking and conspicuous, though not quite so large as her mate. As in the other case, her daughters confirm the pedigree by resembling their more sombre-tinted father in their first plumage, attaining their feminine plumage later on. Readers will be reminded of the similar case in human hair, which is short in babies of both sexes, but afterwards tends to grow longer in women than in men, though the general habit of cutting the men's hair makes the difference seem artificial. The Huia, which in size and shape is not unlike a Jay, was always a bird of limited range, and never found in the South Island ; it is now very local, and it is to be hoped that the State restrictions on the capture of the peculiar birds of New Zealand will be successful in preserving it — without some- thing of the sort it would soon have been exter- minated " in the cause of science." Next to the Huia, the bird which shows most sexual difference in the bill is the Capercailzie, so well known to Scottish sportsmen and a common object in our poulterers' shops in the winter ; and here there is a possible explanation in a different manner of feeding in the two sexes. The cock has a much larger and more hooked bill than the hen, even in proportion to his far greater size ; in fact his bill looks more like that of a bird of 48 BIRD BEHAVIOUR prey than a game-bird. Now, the cock tastes far more of turpentine than the hen, and he is known to feed more on pine and fir-shoots, his mate, with her small ordinary game-bird's beak, seeking her food more upon the ground. This brings us to the consideration of vegetable - feeding birds ; but in the case of these there is seldom much modification of the bill, nearly all the sensational beaks, if we may thus express it, belong- ing to animal-feeding birds. There is, however, a very peculiar specialized beak in a bird which, like the Capercailzie, is especially a haunter of coniferous woods, and derives its food from them — the Cross- bill, in other respects a very ordinary member of the group of Finches. In this bird, often a winter visitor here, and one which has of late years bred quite frequently in Britain, the beak is crossed at the tip, both jaws being curved ; the jaws may cross either to the right or the left, and this crossing is quite accidental or indifferent, having nothing to do with sex. The young Crossbill has, up to the time of leaving the nest, the ordinary Finch- beak shutting evenly ; presumably the direction of the crossing depends on the nestling being right- or left- beaked, i.e. using its beak instinctively more on one side than another. The particular use of the bill is to prise open the scales of pine- and fir-cones, the underlying seeds of which form the Crossbill's favourite food, and are scooped out by the tongue, which is unusually long for a bird of this group. TOUCANS IN EVOLUTION 49 The most striking beaks found among vegetable- feeding birds are of course those of the Toucans and Hornbills, groups which are constantly confused by people unacquainted with birds, and very naturally, since both are so conspicuously over- beaked, so to speak, that the differences between them are quite over-shadowed by their resemblance in the prominent feature, and their general habits are also much alike, both groups being tree-haunters and fruit-eaters, though they take animal food as well. The Hornbills, however, have three toes in front and one behind, the front ones being more or less connected in a common skin, as in Kingfishers, to which they seem to have some affinity ; the Toucans have their toes in pairs, like Woodpeckers, with which they are closely connected by the intermediate family of Barbets (Capitonida). In fact a very interesting evolutionary exhibit might be made up by any museum possessing plenty of specimens of all three families, so as to show the gradation from the Woodpeckers into the Wrynecks, the Barbets with their Crow-like or in some cases almost Wryneck-like bills, and the very unbroken series of Toucans, ranging from forms like the Toucanets (Selenidera), which, in bill and body, hardly exceed the biggest Barbets, of the genus Megal&ma, to the great black, enormous-billed birds of the genus Rbampbastos, the most typical Toucans of all. In the Hornbills there is no gradation into an- other family altogether, but there is much difference 4 So BIRD BEHAVIOUR between the small Magpie-sized Hornbills of the genus Tockus, with little or no " top-story " to the bill, to the large typical Hornbills, often as big as hen Turkeys, with enormous bills generally crowned with a great horny excrescence. The smaller kinds are more insectivorous than the larger, and thus show some approach to the Wood-Hoopoes (Irrisor), which have much more curved bills than the ordinary Hoopoes, and feed when on trees, while the common Hoopoes are ground-feeders. It has been well suggested that the great bills of the larger Hornbills and Toucans are adapted to giving a purchase for wrenching off tough-stalked fruit ; as the birds grew bigger, too, they would not be able to venture on such slender branches, and so would need more to reach out for their food. And if the beak had got merely longer without acquiring bulk, any wrenching effort would have been liable to dislocate it. That Hornbills at any rate work their beaks very hard may be inferred from the facts that in the largest and bulkiest-beaked kinds the edges of the jaws are worn and chipped in elderly specimens, and that if the fruit will not come away by fair means, some Hornbills think nothing of throwing themselves bodily off the bough and wrenching it away by sheer weight. No doubt the effort of recovering their perch in gymnastic exercises like this is what gives these particularly awkward-looking birds the deftness on the wing which many people must have observed when watching them catch in the air grapes or other RHINOCEROS HORNBILLS. Two lorms are shown here, the true Buceros rhinoceros, and the straight-horned Buceros. sylvestris. 50 HANDICAPPING HELMETS 51 food thrown to them in the aviaries in zoological gardens. Even the biggest and most specialized Hornbills and Toucans like animal food, though the ' horn " of the former must often baulk them in obtaining prey from crevices and holes ; for I once had a narrow escape from a nasty dig in the face from a Concave-casqued Hornbill (Dichoceros bicornis) con- fined in a hutch in the Calcutta Bird Market; about half the bill passed between the wooden bars, but the broad helmet acted as an efficient " stop " and spoilt the stroke. Buffon was often fanciful in his remarks on animal structure, but I fancy he was right when he referred to the " horns " of the Hornbills as an actual hindrance in food-getting. It would be interesting to know whether the great helmet only developed after the birds had become more definitely fruit-eaters, or whether the in- convenience of possessing it tended to develop their diet in that direction. There is probably, however, some connection between the adoption of a vegetable diet and large size, for nearly all the biggest birds are either vegetable- or carrion-feeders, i.e. eat food which does not require mobility to obtain it, and can be obtained in bulk if at all, so that, if the digestion permits its assimilation, size is favoured and tends to increase. Reverting to the Woodpecker- Barbet-Toucan alliance, it is interesting to note that the Wood- peckers in spite of their specialization for grub- 52 BIRD BEHAVIOUR hunting under tree-bark, are in many cases fruit- eaters ; our own Greater Spotted Woodpecker, for instance, eats several kinds of fruit, and in North America the Red-headed Woodpecker (Melanerpes erytbrocepbalus) used at any rate to have a very bad name as a fruit- eater, even going so far as to go off with an apple spiked on its bill when leaving the orchard, so says Wilson ; and one has even special- ized in a most peculiar vegetable food, the cambium or inner bark of trees and their sap. This is the Sapsucker (Spbyrapicus varius) of North America, which is often quite a pest by its trick of girdling fruit-trees with its rows of punctures made to ex- tract the sap. The worst of it is that its name and bad reputation have got transferred to other small Woodpeckers ; just as among us the comparatively harmless Kestrel often has to suffer for the misdeeds of the Sparrow-hawk, which really is an inveterate bird-killer, not confining its ravages by any means to Sparrows, but tackling anything from a Blue- tit to a Woodpigeon, so that in its game-list the chicks of Pheasants and Fowls are quite naturally included. I have spoken of the specialization of the Wood- pecker's bill for grub-hunting, but this is not very striking at first sight, and the degree to which the tip of the bill is formed like a chisel is somewhat variable ; it is the hardness of texture that is the important point, and the skill of the bird in using its tool. The Great Black Wroodpecker has been seen in captivity to chip two parallel grooves down CROWS AS WOODCUTTERS 53 an upright post and then prise out the intervening piece, thus going to work in quite a systematic way. It is of interest to find that in the unspecialized Barbets the power of wood-cutting occurs, though apparently only used for hewing out the nest- hole ; but the state of the back of the cage in which a Great Barbet was confined at the Zoo was ample evidence that this bird's powers in this respect are considerable, though the beak is merely Crow-like. The Crows themselves, however, with their usual versatility, can do a good deal in the way of wood-cutting with their stout bills ; Mr. J. Fros- tick, of Balham, once showed me a Carrion-crow he kept as a pet, and its cage afforded ample evidence of the wood-cutting proclivities of its inmate ; and readers of " Barnaby Rudge " will be familiar with the feat of one of Dickens' s real Ravens whose bio- graphies are described in the introduction, though the amount of damage done to the " six stairs and a landing," which he " tore up and swallowed in splinters " is of course humorously exaggerated. The greatest power of wood-cutting is, however, as every one knows, to be seen in Parrots ; it is perhaps for this that their very peculiar bills became so specialized in hardness, shortness, and power, for they mostly build in holes in trees, and are not averse to making holes, either in trees or cliffs, for themselves. Besides, many of them are grub- eaters, and cut away wood in order to obtain their prey, quite taking the place of Woodpeckers in the 54 BIRD BEHAVIOUR Australian region, where these otherwise universally distributed birds are absent. The chief grub- eaters are the Black Cockatoos (Calyptorhynchus) of Australia and the curious New Zealand Parrots known as Kakas (Nestor) ; but the habit is probably commoner than is supposed, since most Parrots in captivity evince a liking for animal food, although too much of this is bad for them, often causing a skin-irritation and the adoption of the feather-plucking habit: This, however, has probably more to do with the want of exercise. It seems improbable that the Parrots should have developed their extraordinary deep-curved bill, unique among vertebrates in its hinged upper jaw and extreme biting power, merely to feed on seeds and fruit, and it is noteworthy that the Parrot which has the least typical beak, Pesquet's Parrot of New Guinea (Dasyptilus pesgueti), in which the upper jaw at any rate is more like an Eagle's than a Parrot's, tears at its food of fruit like a carnivorous bird at flesh, and also has less Parrot-like movements than the rest of its tribe, hopping from bough to bough with a flicking action of the wings — evidently it has not fully attained the Parrot specialization in form or action, and corresponds among the Parrots to the Magpie-Goose among the Ducks. The White Cockatoos, which are generally ground- feeders, use their bills much in digging up bulbs and the egg-cases of locusts ; in the Long- nosed Cockatoos (Licmetis), which are especially addicted to a diet of roots, the upper bill is par- LYRE-BIRDS (MALE AND FEMALE). Thel^yre-bird is the largest of singing-birds, and is one of the few birds which shows decorative plumage without brilliant colours. [Copyright, Hutchinson & Co. 54 IMPERIAL WOODPECKERS. This American bird is the largest of the Woodpeckers, and shows the rare peculiarity of the female's crest being longer than the male's. SCRATCHERS AND SEIZERS 55 ticularly prolonged and forms a most effective hoe. Another hoeing genus, in a very different family, is that of the Monauls (Lopbopborus) among the game- birds, best-known in the person of the splendid " Impeyan Pheasant " ; this bird will continue hoeing by the hour, in search of roots and grubs, and scratches very little, although belonging to a group which are particularly characterized by using the feet in this way in search of food. In fact, the Game-birds used to be known by the general title of Rasores or scratchers, and every one knows what execution they can do in this way. Their scratching instinct and capacity, in fact, is no doubt one of their strongest assets in the struggle for existence ; the habit is not a common one among birds when applied to food-getting, though it crops up again amongst the Passerines ; the Whydah- birds among the Finches, for instance, are scratchers, and so is our Bearded Reedling. The Lyre-bird (Menura superb a), too, is a most powerful scratcher ; indeed, it actually grips clods and throws them back, shifting masses of as much as seven pounds in weight, although not bigger than a Fowl. The feet, however, are only employed in actually seizing food in the birds of prey — Hawks, Eagles, and Owls — and in these are specialized for the purpose, being armed, as every one knows, with particularly long and sharp claws, and having a most powerful grip, at least in the typical forms. There are some interesting minor specializations among these birds ; thus, the especially bird- 56 BIRD BEHAVIOUR killing kinds, such as Sparrow-hawks and Falcons, have very long toes and talons, to give the greatest chance of a grip on an elusive prey seized on the wing. The Falcons usually strike from above, and often kill the prey, when brought down by the talons, with the beak ; the Sparrow-hawks chase and clutch, and kill more slowly by their relentless grip- Rep tile- eating Hawks and Eagles have, on the other hand, particularly short toes, adapted to the usually narrow bodies of their prey, and fish-eaters, of which the Osprey is of course the most typical, have the underside of the toes roughened with little spikes, to give them a secure grip of their slippery quarry. It will be noticed that the Sea- Eagles and the great Fishing-Owls of the East (Scotopelia in Africa, and Ketupa in Asia) have ordinary bare legs, adapted to immersion, whereas ordinary Owls and most of the hunting Eagles are feather-legged. The adaptation here is, however, probably special only in the case of the Owls, for Hawks, which are seldom fishers, are generally bare-legged, as is also the great Harpy-Eagle (Thrasaetus harpy ia), which has far the most power- ful feet of all the birds of prey, the toes being as thick as a man's thumb, and the claws as big as a bear's. The strength of these birds is something enor- mous, and in proportion to their size is greater than that of the quadruped carnivora ; the Goshawk, a giant Sparrow-hawk, can if a strong female (this sex THE PROWESS OF EAGLES 57 being the larger in birds of prey generally, and especi- ally in this typical Hawk group) actually hold and kill a hare, a creature several times its own weight ; and the Indian River-Eagle (Haliaetus leucoryphus), a lighter and less powerful bird than the Golden Eagle, can not only carry off a Greylag Goose, but has been known to strike, lift, and land a fish of a stone in weight, though this quarry taxed its powers to the uttermost, and it could not raise it again when frightened by a shot. In cases like this the impetus of the swoop no doubt counts for a great deal. As for the Golden Eagle, it can even master the wolf, a beast not only far heavier than itself, but terribly armed to boot with most punishing teeth ; it is habitually flown at this quarry by Mongolian magnates, but many birds meet their fate in learn- ing their trade, for after the first grip with one foot is made good, the bird must be quick and dexterous in grappling the beast's face with the other foot to avoid fatal reprisals. The cases in which birds of prey have been found killed by animals of the weasel kind which they had seized are evidently examples of nature's penalty on the bungler, and no doubt exemplify a powerful check on these birds, which otherwise would have few casualties to face except in encounters with each other. The great Owls are not inferior in relative power and ferocity to the Eagles ; a Snowy Owl kept in captivity in Shetland, according to the account of 58 BIRD BEHAVIOUR Saxby, in his book on the birds of that country, once seized upon a cat, and held it in such a powerful grip that had not puss been rescued in time it would have succeeded in its amiable intention of biting her head off. In contrast to these powerful and ferocious species, we find many birds of prey which feed largely on insects, such as the Hobby and Kestrel among the Falcons, and the little Scops Owls among the nocturnal birds of prey ; in both cases the insectivorous forms being closely allied to highly carnivorous ones, for the Hobby is very like a miniature of the arch-bird-killer, the Peregrine, and the Scops Owls are only distinguished by size from the particularly powerful and savage Eagle- Owls of the genus Bubo. Among birds of prey a very near approach to cannibalism occurs in the Barred Owl of North America (Syrnium nebulosum), which has an un- enviable reputation for devouring its smaller rela- tives ; and the Peregrine Falcon frequently preys on the Kestrel. However, this habit is more widely spread than might perhaps be supposed ; the great carrion- eating Petrel known by sailors as the " Nelly " (Ossifraga gigantea) remorselessly devours the smaller Petrels, and the Skua Gull or Bonxie feeds on the Kittiwake Gull. Moreover, the Shrikes, some of which are every bit as carnivorous as the Hawks, are only passerine birds, so when they prey on Finches and Thrushes they also are eating ne.ar relatives ; the Shrike in fact is but a song- CROWS AS EXPERIMENTERS 59 bird modified into a bird of prey, and even yet " mislodging music in a pitiless breast." Unlike the typical true birds of prey, however, it seizes with the bill, not the feet, which differ little from those of ordinary passerine birds. The foot-grasping habit is so rooted in most of the birds of prey proper that even the insectivorous ones catch their small prey with the feet ; it is a common and absurd sight in India when the white- ants or termites are swarming, to see such big birds as Kites (Milvus govinda) catching these tiny things in their raptorial talons, with which they can grip and carry off a rat. There are, however, cases in which the adapta- tion here is not complete ; the Caracara Hawk of America (Polyborus iharus), though seizing birds on the wing with the feet — a feat it seldom performs, by the way — picks up ground-prey with its beak, afterwards transferring it to the feet when in flight, as Mr. Hudson informs us. I have seen the Indian Crow (Corvus splendent) thus transfer an object from bill to feet, as if he wanted to learn the Kite's trick of foot-carrying ; but the object was not food — the Crow is too practical to experi- ment with anything so valuable, and a bit of stick or dry cow-dung was the subject of the experiment. Some very curious specializations in the feeding habits of birds of prey deserve mention here ; those of the Snail - Hawk (Rostrhamus sociabilis) and Bird's - nesting Eagle (Neopus malayensis). The Snail-Hawk is a Buzzard-like, brown bird with a 60 BIRD BEHAVIOUR very long hook to the beak and long talons ; it feeds chiefly on water-snails of the genus Ampullaria, very common in shallow pond-edges in the tropics, but, one would think, very queer food for a Hawk. The long hook of the bill and long talons may be adapted for winkle-pin and shell-grippers respec- tively, but from what will be said directly it is possible that the adaptation may be only apparent. The ways of the Bird's-nesting Eagle are still more strange. This species, from South-eastern Asia, has black plumage and a most peculiar foot, with the claws very long except on the outer front toe, which is quite dwarfed. Its habit is to sail over the tree-tops looking for the nests of small birds, which when found it carries off in its long claws, meanwhile ransacking them for the eggs and young, on which it makes its meal in mid- air. Eating on the wing, by the way, is quite a common accomplishment with birds of prey which capture light quarry ; it was quite the usual thing in Calcutta to see a Kite sailing overhead and tearing at something held in its talons. Once one dropped a bullock's eye almost on me, having no doubt found the tough morsel non-negotiable. Better known than the Snail- Hawk and the Bird's-nester is the Secretary-bird of Africa, which is, indeed, one of the best known of all birds of prey. Most people are familiar with its quaint appearance, as of an Eagle on stilts (I can never see why it should ever have been called the Secretary Vulture) ; the short toes should, however, be THE SECRETARY AND SNAKES 61 noted, and the method of attacking prey, which the keepers at the Zoo will always display with the aid of a dead rat tied to a string. It will be noted that the bird strikes on the prey to kill it, and does not grasp, using one foot at a time, and so quickly that it gets in two or three blows where a man could only give one ; in fact, I am told that the bird will even kill blue-bottles in this way. The wings are kept lifted meanwhile, no doubt in readiness to spring back if the victim retaliates. But in its conflicts with snakes, which have gained it so much notoriety and the protection of our Govern- ment in Africa, it is said to bring the wings into action to beat the reptile down. Its prey, how- ever, is not restricted to snakes, but includes any sort of ground animal it can find and kill ; in fact, Major J. Stevenson-Hamilton, the Game Warden of the Transvaal, says in his fascinating book " Animal Life in Africa," that it is neither more nor less of a snake- eater than other large birds of prey, so that we cannot regard its peculiar specialization as being an adaptation to a reptilian diet. In fact, we may easily make mistakes in trying to correlate structure and function, as may be realized in considering the case of the Kea Parrot (Nestor notabilis) of New Zealand. Notable, indeed, this bird is, not to say notorious, but not in the way its describer meant when he gave it its scientific name. Every one knows now that it has undergone one of the most remarkable changes of habit ever recorded of any animal. Formerly a feeder on 62 BIRD BEHAVIOUR roots, berries, grubs, etc., it soon after the intro- duction of sheep into its haunts in the New Zealand Alps became a most cruel and destructive carnivore, with a ghoulish appetite for live mutton, attacking the sheep at the loins and eating deeply into their flesh. It was at first credited with the desire of devouring the kidneys or the fat surrounding them, but this view appears to be a mistake, the locality of attack being merely the part where the unhappy sheep had no chance of dislodging its tormentor. Of course so extraordinary a habit as this fell under the ban of museum scepticism, but it has been proved up to the hilt. Rewards are paid for the killing of these birds, and, unlike all other New Zealand birds except the Weka Rail (which is destructive to eggs and chickens), it may be sent out of the country. It is a curious thing, however, that specimens sent to the London Zoo soon give up the habit of eating meat and feed on the ordinary seed and other vegetable food given to Parrots ; one lived there some years chiefly on carrots, thus reverting to its natural diet of roots. This would seem to indicate that hunger was the first incentive to the change of diet ; the in- satiable inquisitiveness and destructiveness which are very characteristic of these Parrots would be quite sufficient incentive for them to attack dead sheep and offal, which they did before transferring their attentions, in a natural sequence, to the living animals. They have been known to attack a horse, and the corpse of a man killed by a fall in the EAGLE ATTACKING MAN 63 mountains has also been mutilated by them, so that under certain circumstances of helplessness by accident or illness, they might easily become dangerous, just as rats are in similar circumstances. < It is rare, however, for any birds to attack man for food ; still, this would seem to be the motive for the attacks made on men in the water by Alba- trosses, as in the case where these sea-birds attacked the German sailors in the sea at our defeat of German ships in the sea-fight off the Falklands ; and no one with any wide knowledge of birds could doubt that Eagles probably were in former times, when they were more common and fire-arms less so, very serious enemies to children. It must be remembered that only a very few years ago a case was recorded in the Field in which a Golden Eagle attacked an adult in Scotland, though in this case the motive was apparently revenge, as the man, a gamekeeper, had rescued a Grouse from it earlier on the same day. Hassel- quist, also, in his " Travels in the Levant," published in the eighteenth century, credits an Owl, which he calls Strix orientalis, but which is apparently only the Common Barn- Owl from his description and attribution to it of building- haunting habits, with coming into houses in Syria at night, and destroying babies if not carefully watched. I may say that on taking a little boy of two years old with his parents to see the Owls at the Zoo some years ago I noticed that all the large Eagle- Owls abandoned their usual apathetic unseeing 64 BIRD BEHAVIOUR stare, and glared down earnestly at the child ; and though I could not understand this at the time, on finding Hasselquist's note it struck me that a child-killing Owl might not be at all an impos- sibility. To return to the Kea ; Smith Woodward has suggested, in Mivart's book on birds, that these Parrots used in the days of the existence of the great Emu-like Moas in New Zealand to attack and vivisect these now extinct birds in the same manner as their descendants now do the introduced sheep ; and the possibility of this must of course be borne in mind. The large struthious birds at the present day seem peculiarly helpless against the attacks of smaller birds ; in the Calcutta Zoo Crows have lined a nest of feathers pecked from the back of an Emu, and have pecked sores on the backs of Ostriches. This did not happen in my time, but I have seen a Crow sit on the back of an Ostrich and peck it. If the Kea were thus always more or less carni- vorous, its peculiarities would be easily understood ; it is, as a matter of fact, in gait and movements much more like a Raven than a Parrot, running freely, and hopping frequently, while it is said often to sail on outspread wings when in flight. Its beak also, very long for a Parrot's and with a comparatively gentle curve of the upper jaw, and its comparatively long legs, with its very dull olive-green plumage, suggest a carnivorous bird rather than a Parrot ; but after all, these peculiarities CHANGES IN FEEDING 65 may simply be due to its being, like the eagle- billed Pesquet's Parrot, an unspecialized early form. Its reversion to a vegetable diet in captivity may point the same way ; in any case it is instructive to notice, that, unspecialized as it is, its beak has at any rate a good share of typical Parrot power, judging by its exploits in the way of gnawing wood. I have also seen, when a couple were kept in a cage in the indoor parrot-house at the Zoo, one cracking canary-seed, while the other amused itself by levering up the perch in the next cage hard alongside, for the discomfiture of the inmates. The Kea's is not only the most notorious, but really the most remarkable change of diet known ; but minor changes are often recorded, and though these are often brought about by man's interference in producing a new food-supply or in transporting birds from one country to another, they must not be despised as unworthy of study on that account, since in undisturbed Nature the equilibrium we hear so much about is not constant. Birds colonize areas on their own initiative occasionally, and plants even invade new habitats as circumstances become more favourable for them, so that there are always opportunities for changes of diet. Mr. Hudson has recorded how, since the giant grasses of the Argentine pampas have given place to the turf-making grasses and clovers of Europe, the Chaja or Crested Screamer (Cbauna ckavaria), which used to feed on water-plants, has taken to grazing on land ; it is a curious thing that the 5 66 BIRD BEHAVIOUR fowl-like beak of this bird looks much more suitable for such a diet than for aquatic herbage, for which the broad bills of Swans and Ducks are better fitted ; and in fact the most purely terrestrial Geese show a great reduction in the size of the beak compared with the rest of the family. So that, as in the case of the Kea, the new habit seems quite in conformity with the structure. It has been noted in New Zealand, also, that the native Pigeon (Carpophaga nova-zealandice), natur- ally purely a tree-bird and a feeder on berries and buds, has been found feeding on the ground in rape-fields ; and a similar change has very probably taken place in the habits of our Wood-Pigeon, which in the primitive state .of Europe was probably rnuch more exclusively a feeder on tree-produce than it is to-day, when the grain and root-crops, and the clover of the meadows, tempt it to feed a great deal on the ground ; here it can be seen to be not quite in its element, for it is far less quick in walking than the common Pigeon, a true ground- feeder, and seems not to be able to run, always taking wing when pressed. The Wood-Pigeon, as can be easily observed in our London parks, is a good example of the extra- ordinary gluttony of some birds ; there is some excuse for its greedy feeding when its food consists of herbs, buds, etc., but it keeps up the custom on receiving the public dole of bread and monkey- nuts, which latter it swallows with the husk enclosing them. One evening I began to toss a few of these GLUTTONS AND DRUNKARDS 67 nuts to a Pigeon I encountered just to see how many it would take ; but after eating a couple, it only took the next two or three in its bill and pettishly threw them aside, after which it flew off. Evidently it was already full-fed, but could not resist the temptation to swallow a little more, and its discomfiture reminded me of the story of the glutton Quin, who, after eating all he could of the homely joint of boiled aitchbone of beef, actually burst into tears, after toying with a slice from a roast haunch of venison, which his host, knowing his weakness, had kept in reserve till he should be hors de combat. I have also heard of a case in which some Turkeys kept on one of the Channel Islands obtained access to a garden in which there were mulberry trees, and gorged themselves till they died from the effects of their meal. Birds will even get drunk, or something very like it ; the Jungle-Fowl of Southern India (Callus sonnerati) and of Ceylon (G. lafayetti) feed freely on the fruit of plants of the genus Strobilantbes, which so stupefies them that they can be knocked over with a stick ; and in South Africa Bulbuls (Pycnonotus tricolor) feed on the fermented berries of the Cape gooseberry, which so intoxicate them that they cannot fly straight or far. Many years ago, when a boy at Maidstone, I had an opportunity of seeing that a bird may appear to enjoy the feeling of intoxication. Having read that small birds could be caught by offering them turnip-seed soaked in whiskey, I tried the 68 BIRD BEHAVIOUR experiment, it being winter-time with snow on the ground. Only one bird responded to the alcoholic invitation, a cock Chaffinch, which after partaking was visibly overcome, but able to escape capture; and I retired myself with a feeling of some compunction, fearing he might fall a victim to some cat in his irresponsible condition. But I might have spared my pity ; for next morning, a cock Chaffinch, with ruffled plumage and in an aggressive mood, was finishing the medicated seed and keeping off the Sparrows, who were now quite ready to experiment themselves. After this, I can believe a story which was told me in India by a bird-dealer, to the effect that if you gave a bird an opium pill and then released it, it would come again next day for a repetition of the dose. Of course the Vulture and the Cormorant are re- garded as the stock examples of gastronomical excess in birds ; but I doubt if this is just. They cer- tainly can perform phenomenal gorging feats, but it must be remembered that the Cormorant must at any rate search and work hard for its food, and that the Vulture's meals are very intermittent. From what I saw in India, where Vultures were practically always on view aloft at some time of the day and somewhere, while one could seldom see them feeding, I came to the conclusion that they probably only got a meal about once a week, so that if they indulged heavily at such times they could not fairly be- accused of gluttony. It will be UNAPPETISING RATIONS 69 noticed that in confinement, where Vultures are fed regularly, they do not by any means eat im- moderately ; a piece of meat the size of a good big steak will suffice for the daily meal of a bird as big as a Turkey. Mr. Beebe, the Bird Curator of the New York Zoo, found that Vultures in captivity preferred fresh to tainted meat, and an attempt made some years ago to feed the Vultures on entrails instead of flesh at our London establishment did not meet with success, so that it is pretty obvious that these birds in a state of nature are often forced by hunger to consume substances which they do not really appreciate. The extreme case of this is that of the small White Scavenger Vultures (Neophron) which habitually feed on excrement, not being strong enough to contend with the larger Vultures for carrion ; Tristram in Palestine saw them looking on wistfully while Griffon Vultures tore at a carcase, to which they eagerly rushed as soon as the big birds retired at his approach, only to be driven off when their tyrants deemed it safe to come back again. It is well known, too, that hawthorn- berries form an important part of the food of our familiar birds of the Thrush tribe, and of Wood-Pigeons ; yet during several London winters, which have been mild, I have noticed that haws hung on the trees till spring, untouched by the numerous Blackbirds, Thrushes, and Wood-Pigeons that fre- quent Regent's Park, showing that these berries are 70 BIRD BEHAVIOUR really emergency rations, which are seldom touched so long as more palatable food is obtainable. Holly- berries are known to be even less favoured, while on the other hand the berries of the mountain- ash are soon eaten up, and Mr. Hudson has described Missel-Thrushes coming to yew-trees on the downs again and again, disgorging these berries half- digested in order to return to the feast, a piece of depravity only to be compared to the practices of the gluttons of ancient Rome, who were wont to go out to be sick in the middle of dinner. Most observant people have seen, too, how the Starlings strip the elders of their- berries as soon as these are ripe, and in India I found their relatives, the various Mynahs, equally attentive to the berries of the peepul fig (Ficus religiosa), which were also attractive to the Barbets ; the Bulbuls especially revelled in the berries of the introduced Lantana shrub, though in winter I have seen them eating buds, though with no goodwill, the buds being evidently .only taken in default of better food. The tree most attractive to birds I have ever seen, however, is the whitebeam (Pyrus aria), at any rate in the case of a specimen which used to stand close to the South Gate of the Zoological Gardens before the new Eagle Aviary was built. When this was hung with its large orange haw-like berries, it was constantly frequented by Starlings (which certainly do not eat ordinary haws if they can help it), Blackbirds, and Wood- Pigeons, all intent on feeding on the fruit as long as it lasted ; WORMS NOT APPRECIATED 71 I once even saw a Moorhen in the top of the tree, and although I did not actually see it feeding, unless it was there after the fruit I do not know what its business was, there being a much more natural haunt for it in the rushes and shrubbery of the Three-island Pond a few yards away. The traditional diet of worms is probably not so entirely to the taste of birds as one would sup- pose ; at any rate, on keeping a wild-caught Missel- Thrush in a cage I noticed that it would not come down from its perch for an earth-worm unless I stood well away, while a mealworm (an indoor- living beetle-grub which it could hardly come across naturally) would bring it to the floor at once. A Song-Thrush also I knew in India and supplied with gentles would not eat the local earthworms. I may say these had a very peculiar faint, sickly smell ; but on the other hand I cer- tainly rarely saw wild birds eating these — the only instances I can remember of seeing a bird with a worm in its possession being those of a Brown Shrike (Lanius cristatus) — a bird very like the hen of our Red-backed Shrike; the White-breasted Kingfisher (Halcyon smyrnensis), which is more a hunter than a fisher; and the common Babbling- Thrush (Crateropus canorus), which I have often seen, on the other hand, pecking away at the earth- galleries of the termites. When these " white- ants " swarm, they furnish a free supper for a great variety of insectivorous birds, notably the Crow (Corvus sflendens), Kite 72 BIRD BEHAVIOUR (Milvus govinda), King-Crow (Dicrurus ater), and Roller (Coracias indie a). Similarly one can see the swarming of the true ants greatly appreciated by our Sparrows and Starlings, the latter especially capturing them with almost Swallow-like skill ; while on the other hand I have seen a Swallow reverse its natural habits by settling to pick these ants up off the ground. The most extraordinary aberration of feeding- habit I ever witnessed, however, was the behaviour of a Song-Thrush I watched at Oxford some years ago, as it was running along the edge of the stream that flows through Magdalen College grounds ; suddenly it dashed into the water and secured one of a shoal of minnows, which it beat on the ground and then swallowed whole. It is probable that fish are often eaten by land-birds when the opportunity presents itself, as they certainly are by water-birds not usually reckoned as fishers ; thus, I have seen a Coot I kept in India suddenly duck its head under and secure a little fish — and it must be remembered that to an insectivorous bird anything will serve for an insect if it is small enough. In India I have seen the House-Mynah (Acri- dotheres tristis) with a gecko lizard in its bill, and in Africa the exquisite Malachite Sun-bird (Nec- tarinia famosa), a nectar-eater by custom and structure, has been found to have fed on tiny lizards ; while in Britain the Missel-Thrush has not only been found preying on the young of the HIGH-FLAVOURED DAINTIES 73 Hedge-Sparrow, but even carrying off a young Song- Thrush — a feat nearer actual cannibalism than any of those recorded above of birds usually reckoned as predatory. Predatory birds of all families, by the way, gener- ally agree in their liking for mice ; and these " smelly " little rodents are likewise much appreciated by omni- vorous feeders. Fowls and Ducks will swallow them, and in experiments on the feeding of the common Crow of America (Corvus americanus), in captivity the experimenters found that it appeared to be impossible for a Crow to be so full that he would refuse a mouse, and that timid new-caught speci- mens would crowd to the front of the cage to seize one. And we all know the service that Gulls and Rooks, as well as Owls and Hawks, did in the vole plague in Scotland a few years ago. American investigators have also found that Crows especially affect, as investigation of stomach- contents shows, strong-smelling beetles, so that it is not safe to put down an insect as unpalatable simply because it smells nasty to us. In India I have found the great and very foul-odoured cock- roach (Periplaneta americana), an exotic and usually an indoor insect, was readily accepted not only by the Racket- tailed Drongo (Dissemurus paradiseus) and Babbling-Thrush in captivity, but also by the Brown Shrike outside, while I saw Kites and Sparrows also feed on casual specimens they had obtained. It is perhaps rather significant, however, that 74 BIRD BEHAVIOUR just as birds particularly readily eat termites, which are used as human food in Africa, so they are also particularly fond of grasshoppers and locusts, which are and have been more widely used as human food than any other insects, being even permitted to the Jews by the Mosaic law. Jerdon, whose obser- vations on Indian birds have never been superseded, though often amplified, considered that grass- hoppers were the staple food of insectivorous birds in India, and the observations of American investi- gators have shown that they are equally appreciated by the birds of North America. When locust-swarms invade a country, they are preyed on wholesale by a great variety of birds ; in the case of the only swarm — a very scattered one — I ever saw in India, it was amusing to see the Calcutta Sparrows, who probably had in few cases ever seen a locust before, valiantly tackle these huge shrimp-pink grasshoppers on the wing and bring them to the ground. Certain special kinds of birds also have long been known as the particular enemies of locusts, following them everywhere, and thus earning the gratitude of humanity ; of these the most noteworthy being the Rosy Pastor (Pastor roseus), the most beautiful of the Starlings, in Eastern Europe and in Asia, and the Pratincoles or Swallow- Plovers (Glare ola) in Africa, where also another Starling (Dilopbus carunculatus) is a well-known " Locust-bird." Dragon- flies are also greatly appreciated by birds ; I have seen two very different species using them BUTTERFLIES AS BIRD DIET 75 to feed their young in our Museum grounds in Calcutta — the Red-eared Bulbul (Otocompsa emeria) a fruit-eating tree-bird, and the Indian Dabchick (Podicipes capensis). The victims of the Bulbuls were always, so far as I saw, the slender small Agrionid types ; so were the Dabchicks* generally, and it was amusing to see the little divers sneak along with head lowered to the surface, and capture the resting insect with a sudden spring. I once saw a bright scarlet Libellulid offered to a young Dabchick, and once saw a Brown Shrike capture one of these red dragon-flies in repose. It is at such times, I fancy, that these most active of insects probably fall victims, and the same remark would apply to flies. Quick as these are on the wing, I have seen the heavy, awkward-looking Muscovy Drake (Cairina moschata) waddle up and pick them off leaves with ease and certainty, To birds which are specialized for catching insects flying, such as Swifts, Swallows, etc., of course no flying insects present any difficulty, but it is significant that birds of this type, whose preying habits are so well known and conspicuous, rarely seem to attack butterflies. No bird is known as the " butterfly-catcher " anywhere, though we have " bee-eaters " and " fly-catchers," and though moths are ravenously pursued, as one may see even in London with the Sparrow. This is not to say that birds never eat butterflies, but that these do not form a common prey ; in India I certainly did not see a bird attack or possess one oftener than 76 BIRD BEHAVIOUR once a year on the average, though particularly on the look-out for this, and the American investiga- tions on the food of birds showed that in 40,000 stomachs of insectivorous birds four butterflies only were found. CHAPTER IV Nutrition (continued) — Manipulation of food — Powers of digestion, differing in different groups — The formation of pellets or castings — Difference in the food of old and young in some cases — Different methods of feeding the young — Young assisting parents in feeding their juniors — Feeding of each other by the sexes — Drinking, and eating of such substances as salt and earth. GENERALLY speaking, there is little manipulation of food among birds, it being generally swallowed whole ; and in many cases the power of deglutition displayed is almost as remarkable as in the case of reptiles. Fish-eating birds have the greatest repu- tation for bolting huge morsels, but some of the vegetable-feeding groups are very good seconds, especially the large fruit-eating Pigeons, some of which are instrumental in disseminating the nutmeg by swallowing it for the sake of the investing " mace." In accordance with this habit of wholesale swal- lowing, the tongue is of little importance in most birds ; and although it is generally well-developed, it is very rarely protruded beyond the bill, and generally lies nearly inert within the lower jaw. In the Pelicans and their allies, Gannets, Cormorants, 77 78 BIRD BEHAVIOUR and Snake-birds (Plotus), it is a mere pimple-like rudiment, as it is also in the curious Shoe-billed Stork (Bal&niceps rex) ; it is tempting in this case to put its disappearance down to the necessity for clearing the course for swallowing large prey, but it is very doubtful if this explanation is correct, for a very short tongue, if not an actually rudimentary one, occurs in groups with different feeding-habits, while both long and short tongues are found in groups where the food is the same. Thus, the animal-feeding Kingfishers and the mainly fruit-eating Hornbills both have very short tongues ; the Herons have long tongues, the Storks short ones — both animal- feeders. Among vegetable- feeders, the Toucans, though so like the Hornbills in their form and habits, especially with regard to the bill and its use, have long tongues ; the Part- ridges have tongues of suitable length for their beaks, the Tinamous, so like them in feeding and general habits, very short ones, like their giant relatives of the flightless Ostrich tribe, all of which are short-tongued, including the small worm-eating Apteryx. It would seem, therefore, that in most cases the possession of a tongue proportionate to the bill or a very short one is a group-character, and that the organ is in a state of degeneration in many cases. I may mention, however, that in two cases I have seen birds indulge in the curious — for them — action of licking their chops ; in that of the Heron (Ardea cinerea) and the Pied Kingfisher (Ceryle varia), both TONGUES AS FOOD-GETTERS 79 fishers, but one long-tongued and the other short, though the beaks are so much alike. In some cases, however, the tongue may be useful and even of vital importance ; we have seen it is so in the Crossbill, and in the Woodpeckers in the various honey-feeding groups it is most instrumental in getting the food. Woodpeckers have the tongue very long and wormlike, and can protrude it to a distance at least equal to the length of the bill ; the tip is horny and barbed along the sides, so that the organ not only acts as a probe but a grapple, and is used to drag out grubs, etc., that cannot be reached with the bill. In the Sapsucker above-mentioned the armature of the tip is slightly modified, so that it is more like a brush than a spear-head. I found in young specimens of the Indian Golden-backed Wood- pecker (Brachypterus aurantius), which I reared from the nest, that the end of the tongue is plain until the bird is fledged, the barbs not appearing till later ; one very tame one I had gave tactile demonstrations of the use of the tongue as a probe, for she used not only to sound the top of my head with her bill in a most uncomplimentary way, but tickle my ears as if they were so many worm-holes. In our Wryneck the protrusibility of the tongue and its use in feeding on ants and their cocoons has long attracted notice ; the quickness of action in this case is such that the ants' cocoon appears to the observer to be attracted as by a magnet ; in the bigger and slower-moving Woodpecker the 8o BIRD BEHAVIOUR worm-like tongue is conspicuous enough. No doubt the apparent attraction of the Wryneck's food to its bill, and the bird's queer snake-like movements under excitement, were what commended it to ancient sorcery as a love-charm ; it was, as students of the classics know, bound to a wheel, which was spun as a love-charm, the " iynx " being invoked to bring the beloved person to the spinner. The custom, indeed, became proverbial ; " to spin the Wryneck " at any one was the Greek equivalent for our " setting one's cap " at him. To turn from Wrynecks and witches to Wood- peckers ; the extensile tongue is brought into play by others than the Sapsucker to procure vegetable juices ; a Woodpecker in Jamaica (Me- lanerfes striolatus) makes itself very objectionable to planters by puncturing the rind of the sugar- cane and sucking out the juice ; and another in Cuba (M. superciliaris) is such a pest by performing a similar feat in the orange-groves that it is, or used to be, the custom to turn the army on to Woodpecker- shooting when there was not a revolu- tion on hand to keep them healthily occupied. One of this species was some time ago in the pos- session of Mr. J. D. Hamlyn, the well-known bird- dealer, and used to amuse itself by using its tongue to grapple the tails of some Budgerigars or Grass- Parrakeets in the compartment immediately above it. These sap- and juice-sucking Woodpeckers natur- ally remind one of the true nectar-feeding birds, SYRUP-SIPHONS IN ACTION 81 of which the Humming-birds of America are far the best known ; and it is a curious and interesting fact that the Humming-birds and Woodpeckers both have the tongue supported on C-springs, for in both the horns of the " hyoid " bone, which supports the base of the tongue, are immensely long and slender, and curve right over the back of the head. This is, however, only one of the cases in which a common internal character is no more important than a common external one, for there is no reason to suppose from the rest of their structure that the Humming-birds and Wood- peckers are related ; and the tongue itself is quite different in the Humming-birds, for though just as protrusible as the Woodpecker's tongue, it is composed of two horny tubes lying parallel, each side of the tongue curling inward, and the whole forming a suction-pump most efficacious in sucking up honey from flowers. Syrup-sucking is only a side-line with the Wood- peckers, but with Humming-birds it is certainly the main business in many cases, though all eat insects more or less, and some, such as the plain- coloured " Hermit " group (Phaethornis and allies) do not visit flowers at all, but feed only on insects, for which they search the trunks of trees and the undersides of leaves, always hovering, however, just as the flower-feeding species do before the flowers. That Humming-birds are essentially syrup- drinkers is shown by the fact that they come readily to glasses of artificial syrup put out for them, and 6 82 BIRD BEHAVIOUR when newly captured will readily drink up syrup while held in the hand. So will the Sun-birds of the Old World — at any rate the two Asiatic species of which I have had experience, the Purple (Cin- nyris asiatica) and the Amethyst-rumped (C. zeylonica). These small and brilliantly coloured birds are often mistaken for Humming-birds, just as the Hornbills of the East are often called Toucans, the American birds in each case having obtained the earlier and greater reputation with the public at large, as well as with naturalists. And indeed the habits of the Toucans and Hornbills are sufficiently alike to afford a good excuse for this ; but the Sun-birds, charming little creatures though they are, seem commonplace after the fairy-like hovering Humming-birds, since they simply hop about like Tits or small Warblers, and do not hover more than those birds do. Their tongue is also less specialized, though also tubular, but their diet is exactly the same, and Mr. A. Ezra, who had at the time of writing the only two living Humming- birds in this country — one specimen each of the Garnet- throated (Eulampis jugularis) and of Ricord's (Sporadinus ricordi) — finds that they thrive well on the same fortified syrup, composed of honey, Mellin's baby-food, and condensed milk, which has well served the various species of Sun-birds for which he first used it. Neither Sun-birds nor Humming-birds can support existence for long on a diet of sugar-syrup alone, BOURU FRIAR-BIRD, The head in this large Honey-eater is feathered, but the more typical Friar-birds are bald-headed, whence the name given them. 82 . s. Q 2 g £ o 5 I £ s o £ W including Cormorants and Gannets, matters go much further ; at any rate the young bird does, for it gets its head and even neck well down the long- suffering parent's gullet. I well remember in Calcutta getting hold of a fledging young nestling of the Darter or Snake- bird, which was brought to the market for sale half-reared. It was charmingly tame, but the head, at the end of its long slender neck, wobbled most provokingly when I tried to feed it, so that, eager though it was for the fish I offered, it was a difficult matter to get one placed in its mouth. The obvious move that suggested itself was of course to hold the wobbling head in one's hand to steady it ; and to my intense surprise, as soon as I did this, the bird's mouth suddenly expanded to about twice its previous size, so that it looked like a funnel. Obviously the clasp of my hand reproduced the pressure of the parent's gullet, to which the nestling instinctively responded. Since then I have read an interesting note by Miss R. Alderson, a keen amateur of Doves, about her difficulties in getting food into the mouth of a very young Dove-nestling she was rearing, until its beak happened to slip between her fingers, when it opened it at once, and was thereafter fed quite easily ; evidently in this case a little pressure to represent the feel of the parent's bill was all that was needed. SPOILT CUCKOO CHILDREN 119 The method of putting the beak and often a good deal more, as we have seen, inside the parent's mouth, is, however, not the usual one with nestlings which are fed by the parents ; the more popular method is for the young to gape and the parents to place the food in their open beaks, whether they disgorge it or bring it in morsels. This may be seen in the case of Canaries, Sparrows, and small birds generally, to say nothing of less familiar creatures like Hawks, Owls, Kingfishers, Rollers, etc. Nightjars grip the bill of their parent with their own, and young Storks, judging from the behaviour of specimens which are reared now and then at the Zoo, expect the old birds to throw up the food into the nest, when they pick it up and swallow it — a most insanitary-looking proceeding. Generally speaking, nestling birds have no idea of picking anything up till they are nearly fledged, sometimes not till after they can fly, and would starve in the midst of plenty through not dreaming of stooping to pick up their food. Cuckoos are particularly slow in learning to pick up food, and in nature seem to be thoroughly spoilt by their foster-parents. They are especially ravenous, and this applies to the non-parasitic " Crow- Pheasant " of India, as well as to the parasitic species. Even among our few domesticated birds we are well acquainted with two different types of young, which may be conveniently called active and passive, the " nidifugous " and " nidicolous " young of ornithologists ; the latter, about which I have 120 BIRD BEHAVIOUR been speaking above, being exemplified by the " nestling " young of Canaries, Budgerigars, Doves, and Pigeons, while our ordinary poultry, with their active running or swimming chicks, exemplify the former. When we study the bird class family by family, we shall find that the cleavage between these two types of nestlings is very definite and complete ; that is to say, they never both occur in the same family. All young Pigeons, so far as is known, are as helpless as those of the common Pigeon, even in the case of those Ground-Doves found in America and Australia which so closely resemble Partridges in general habits ; and all young of the Duck family, from the grazing Geese to the fishing Mergansers, are as well able to "paddle their own canoe" as the familiar domestic duckling. The two types crop up independently, moreover, in very distinct families ; no one would think of classing Pigeons and Hawks together because they both have helpless young and feed them, or putting Fowls and Ducks in the same group because in both cases the young are active and more or less inde- pendent. It will be seen that the two types of young have no relation to the general habits of the parents, whether carnivorous or vegetarian, terres- trial or aquatic ; the production of one or the other type may be regarded as a minor habit, generally connected with the style of nest or nesting-site used. Thus, birds which have a well- developed perching foot, such as Pigeons, Herons, PASSIVE AND ACTIVE YOUNG 121 Cormorants, and Parrots, to say nothing of the numerous tribe of Passerines, generally build above ground on bushes, trees, or rocks ; and such will naturally enough have helpless young ; those which have the foot only formed for running or swimming, and the hind-toe very small or absent, are naturally less apt to perch, build on the ground as a rule, and have young which are active runners or swimmers. It thus becomes possible in most cases to predict the probable nature of a previously unknown bird's young by the structure of the adult's foot ; but such detective-story methods in natural history are to be employed with great caution, and in the present case, as might be expected, exceptions occur. To take the flightless birds, for instance, the rule works out all right with the flightless runners, from Ostrich to Apteryx, all of which have non- grasping feet with the hind-toe absent in all but the last ; but it fails when we turn to the Penguins, which have a dwarfed useless hind- toe, even situated on the inner side of the foot, and yet have young which are helpless nestlings, in spite of the labour thus entailed on the flightless parents in trudging to and from the sea to get supplies for them, a labour in which the male is as earnest as the hen. Among the flying birds, too, we find that in the Petrels, which are by no means perchers, having the hind toe reduced to a nail, and breed more often in holes in the ground than anywhere else, that the young are helpless and fed with an oily substance disgorged into their mouths by the parents. 122 BIRD BEHAVIOUR Then on the other side, there is at least one family of tree-haunting and high-nesting birds which has active young, that of the Curassows and Guans (Cracidce), perching game-birds with gripping feet, which take the place of Pheasants and their kin in the forests of the warm parts of America. The chicks here are fed by the parent for a day or so at any rate, judging from the behaviour of captive specimens, and they are hatched with the wing- feathers as well developed as in a chicken a fortnight old ; it seems that they follow the parents along the boughs and creeping vines — so I was told by a naturalist, who said the chicks could be caught by shaking them down ; no doubt this is one reason why so many of these birds are so extraordinarily tame in captivity ; they have presumably been hand-reared after such a method of capture. Some of the typical game-birds, for instance the Javan Peacock (Pavo muticus), the Tragopans, and also the Argus, have the quills well developed at birth, so that there is not such a great gap between more ordinary game-birds and the young of the Megapodes or Mound-builders, which are hatched with wings quite full-fledged and fit for immediate flight, though the body is downy. These represent the extreme of precocity in modern birds, and are the most independent of all young ones, since the parents take no trouble about them at all, as, being able both to feed and to fly, as well as to run, all they need to gain is experience and size in order to be as good as any adult. WINGS FIRST TO FEATHER 123 The Game-birds, then, typify one method of fledging, which is found among few others than them, that of the wings developing long before the rest of the plumage. Even our domestic chickens show this well, although not " able to fly in a few hours," as a much-respected authority stated. Chicks of this type pick up most of their food, though the hen often draws their attention to it ; young Peafowl and Turkeys do not begin to feed for a longer period after hatching than Fowls, a peculiarity which is no doubt the reason why poultry-keepers stigmatize Turkey-chicks as stupid, and needing to be " taught to peck " — the real reason being that the yolk in the intestines at the time of hatching lasts them longer than it does Fowl-chicks, though even these need nothing for the first twenty-four hours. Besides the Game-birds, Hemipodes, and Tina- mous, the Rhea and the common Cape Penguin (Spheniscus demersus) exhibit the peculiarity of fledging first on the wings. At the other extreme of precocity comes the Fowl's domestic companion, the Duck ; ducklings are even more lively than chickens, need less brooding and for a shorter period, and are more enterprising in foraging for food, on land as well as in what is popularly regarded as their only proper element. In fact, a poultry-breeder once complained to me that ducklings of the Indian Runner breed simply ran away from the old hens who were acting as foster-mothers, who could not keep up with them, i24 BIRD BEHAVIOUR young ducklings being notoriously insubordinate, at any rate to hens, though they will listen to a Duck mother if she calls to them. But in spite of their independence, which is greater than that of any young birds other than the Megapodes, the fledging of young Ducks is most curiously slow. They grow and grow, and lose all their chubby infantile prettiness, and get more than half as big as their parents before any feathers come at all ; and even when these do appear — on the shoulders first, and then the sides of the breast — the wings remain very tiny, out of all proportion to the powerful legs, and do not enlarge and sprout quills till the bird is full-feathered almost every- where else and is nearly full-sized ; this is the stage when the birds are known as flappers. Geese and Swans also develop in exactly the same way. Some other water- or marsh-birds show a similar slowness in growing feathers to replace the down, and in the enlargement and quill-growth of the wings ; these are the Rails — as any one can see in the case of the young of the common Moorhen and Coot — the Cranes and the Grebes. Young Grebes have a special remarkable peculiarity of their own in fledging ; the head-feathering " hangs fire " longest of all the plumage, so that a Grebe may be full- winged and downy-headed at the same time, a very rare case among birds, though a similar one is found among the young of the Cranes. So far as is known, this retarded fledging, like the precocious fledging, runs through the family wher- EVEN GROWTH OF FEATHERS 125 ever it occurs ; it will be noticed that except in the case of the Penguin, these methods of fledging are only found in families which have active chicks. The third method of fledging is that which occurs in the vast majority of birds, and is what one would have reasonably expected in all of them ; that is to Say, the feathers grow pretty evenly all over the body, and the wings develop in size like the legs, and grow their quills pretty nearly as quickly as the body-feathers, and much more so than the tail, which goes ahead in the case of the retarded fledgers of the Duck and Rail type ; our Pigeons, Doves, Canaries, and Budgerigars furnish familiar examples of this, as well as such wild birds as Thrushes, Sparrows, etc. But these are all birds with helpless young ; and the uniform method of fledging, being so prevalent, is also found among birds whose chicks are active, such as Plovers and Gulls. Some people, indeed, regard the young Gull as intermediate between the active and passive type of young bird ; but as it is able to walk about, even when quite small, and can pick up the food the parent vomits for it, even if it does not provide for itself, it can fairly claim to be put on the active list. It is, however, often hatched in a nest which puts pedestrian exercise out of the question, as when this is on a ledge of a cliff, and so there is certainly a tendency in young Gulls to degenerate towards the passive type. This is so also with their diving relatives the Auks, but even these, cliff- 126 BIRD BEHAVIOUR dwellers though they are, can walk, and pick up fish, etc., where brought by the parents — which do not swallow and vomit it, but carry it in the bill — and in the case of the Guillemot, at any rate, go down to the sea when half-fledged, their fledging being uniform as in Gulls, though this premature jump into existence results in the death of many. The young of that most curious bird the Cariama (Cariama cristata) also seems to show a partially degenerate type. It lies quietly in the nest, which is built aloft, though the old birds spend most of the time on the ground, but yet has patterned down, dark brown with a cream streak down each side of the back and cream spots upon the brown ; and as such down is usually found in runners, it presents to the eye a queer blend of the two types of young ; but the young one I observed, bred at the Zoo, left the nest long before it could possibly fly, though perhaps this was premature. At any rate it climbed back to it when a bough was placed as a ladder, so the same thing may occur in nature, the nest being in a tree ; it did not seek for food, but waited till the old birds gave it, just as Moor- hens and Coots do when they are very young ; and they keep taking it from the parents at times even when fledged, as this young Cariama did, and thus show, on their part also, signs of pauper degeneracy. In fact, the feeble, whining squeak and imploring attitudes of the young of the Rail tribe contrast very unfavourably with the liveliness of their frequent companions, the ducklings ; in the case ENTERPRISING DUCKLINGS 127 of the New Zealand Weka Rail, the parents, which are flightless ground-birds, have been seen running to and fro until quite weary, plying with food their young which they had left in a place of safety. Grebes, again, nursed on the parent's back and fed there or on the nest for a week or so, come very near being mere nestlings, and are very feeble at first, unable to do more than crawl, and un- willing to swim ; and the Swans' cygnets are not nearly so active on land as goslings and ducklings, though downy and swimming well enough ; and as their parents carry them on their backs, at any rate at times, and do something to feed them — the Mute Swan, as is well known, pulling up weeds for them, while the Black Swan, as I have seen, will pull them grass from the bank — these young also seem to approach nearer the nest-fostered type than the active majority of the family. Indeed, the young of several members of this group are much more active and enterprising than their parents ; young Mallard frequently dive for food like young Tufted Ducks and Pochards, even up to the flapper-stage, though adults very seldom do so, and they are also very much more active on land ; young Sheldrakes of three species that I have watched, the Common (Tadorna vulpanser), Ruddy (Gas arc a rutila), and New Zealand (C. variegate), dive most freely for food during the first week or two, while I have never seen the adults of any of these species do this, while they readily turn tail-up in the usual Duck manner, although in the ordinary 128 BIRD BEHAVIOUR way they are more land-feeders than Ducks gener- ally. So are common Wigeon; and I have seen their young ducklings dive for food, but, curiously enough, not Mandarin ducklings, though the old birds will dive for food more readily than any other surface-feeding Duck — indeed, I have seen them do so more freely than at least two of the diving Ducks — the Rosy-billed (Metopiana peposaca) and Red-crested Pochard (Netta rufina). What habits the young of the curious Magpie- Goose may have do not seem to have been recorded ; it would be interesting to know if they fledge late like the rest of the family. Flamingoes, by the way, show a distinct difference from the Ducks in this respect; they fledge early and gradually, the wing quills developing in good time, just like the young of Storks and Ibises, to which, as I said above, they appear really to be allied. Their young are fed with disgorged food from the beak, at any rate at first, just as in these members of wading groups. Quite the most curious instance of parental aid given to active young birds is that of the Wood- cock, which carries its young from the dry wood in which they are hatched to the moist feeding- grounds, gripping them between the legs, which are in this bird very short for a wader, not longer, in fact, than a Partridge's. Even when the young are so far advanced towards maturity as to be bigger than a Snipe, the habit is kept up. Another instance of active young being carried RESISTANCE TO INJURY 129 in the parent's feet is that of the young of a very common Eastern water-fowl, the Small Whistling Tree-Duck (Dendrocycna javanica), which commonly breeds in trees, and has been seen to bring the young 'ducklings down in that way. The tree- breeding habit occurs frequently in Ducks, espe- cially in tropical climates, and our Wild Duck frequently breeds in pollard willows or on decayed stumps, but the young in such cases generally jump down and take their chance, though M. Rogeron records one case in which he saw the old bird carry down the young in her bill, as the American Wood-Duck or Carolina Duck (&x sponsa) is said to do. I have heard, however, of a case in which the nearly related Mandarin Duck, breeding at large on an English estate, nested in a hole about fifty feet above a hard carriage drive, and the young simply jumped down and sprinted off for the water ; of course young Ducks are very light, and thickly padded with stiff down, and so can take risks of the kind better than might be supposed. But the power of resistance of even adult birds of such a kind to a fall must be great ; I have known an adult Carolina Duck escape from the former zoological sale-collection on the terrace at Covent Garden and fall (being pinioned) on the hard stones beneath, and yet not be injured. A curious instance of a young bird of the active type in appearance being nevertheless a nestling in habits is that of the young of the South American 9 130 BIRD BEHAVIOUR Sun-Bittern (Euryfyga belias) which bred in the London Zoo at the zenith of that institution, when it was under the Sclater and Bartlett management, and really excellent results were got there. This bird is .described as being like a young Woodcock or Snipe in appearance, with variegated down, and it did not gape or cry like young birds generally, but when the parents flew on to the nest, which was at the top of a pole, with a small fish or other food in their bills, it snapped or pecked the morsel from them and devoured it. The Sun-Bittern is not well-named, for, in spite of its carnivorous and wading habits, it is not at all like a Bittern, and forms a family of its own, related to the Rails ; evidently it is a bird whose young are degenerating into the nestling type. Within this type are to be found, as will be in- ferred from what has been said, varying degrees of degeneracy ; young birds of prey, for instance, can seize with their feet and tear up food before they are out of the down. They will even walk about a little, if their surroundings permit, while in this stage, and so will young Penguins and even Pelicans, though in the case of the latter there is often little chance, as these birds as often as not nest on trees. Young Nightjars are also quite able to run, and do so for short distances. Young Larks may be perhaps returning to an active condition, as they will ramble away from the nest before fledging ; Mr. W. Farren has amusingly described in a recent number of Wild NON-SLIP PADS FOR NESTLINGS 131 Life how young Woodlarks venture away for a few feet from the nest when alone and undisturbed, and then, when frightened, shrink back to it, as if attached to invisible strings of elastic which pulled them back by contraction. This tendency to " reverse engines " is very marked among many nestlings, and is no doubt connected partly with the habit of backing to the edge or opening of the nest in order to discharge their dung, a piece of instinctive sanitation, and with retirement on alarm without turning round. It is particularly well marked in young King- fishers, which run backwards in a most comical way. They are provided with a pad on the hock- joint, on which they rest, like so many young birds, in repose ; but it is not nearly so well developed in them as in young Woodpeckers and Barbets, which have a very marked horny- studded heel- cap, and, as nestlings, do not move on their toes at all, but on the heels, as I was, I believe, the first to record, having reared the Indian Gold-backed Woodpecker (Bracbypternus aurantius) and two In- dian Barbets — the Blue-throated (Cyanops asiatica) and the Crimson-breasted (Xaniholezma bama- tocephala)' — from the nest, and so noted not only the occurrence of the heel-pad (which had been previously observed in the Wryneck), but also its function. Such a pad has since been observed by Mr. Seth-Smith in the young of a Toucanet (Seleni- dera maculirostris). These young Barbets, by the way, had a pecu- 132 BIRD BEHAVIOUR liarity I never observed in any other nestlings : they kept their tails turned over on their backs, and under their closed wings ; a most admirable ar- rangement for the tight packing which is the lot of nestlings reared in holes hacked out in trees, as young Barbets are, but nevertheless young Wood- peckers have not the habit, though similarly placed. The nestling most celebrated for its peculiarities is that of the Hoatzin (Opisthocomus hoazin) of the forests along the edge of some rivers in northern South America. The adult bird is not unlike a perching Pheasant in general form, size, and habits, but the young are most curious creatures, clad at first in a scanty down like that of a young Pigeon, but nevertheless born open-eyed, not blind as Pigeons .and other naked or nearly naked nestlings are ; they are also able to clamber about the boughs, in which action not only the bill and feet are brought into play, but also the wings, which have claws at the end of the first and second digits, the first digit in birds being that little movable point which supports the set of pigmy quills known as the bastard- wing, which may be seen projecting at the pinion- joint of a Pigeon's wing when it is about to settle, and the second forming the tip of the pinion. These claws of course represent two of the three original claws found in the separate digits of the primaeval birds' forelimb (which were probably simply enclosed in a common skin like a Kingfisher's toes), and so it is interesting to find them still in YOUNG HOATZIN. A half-fledged specimen, showing its method of climbing with bill, wings, and feet. 132 BRUSH-TURKEY. This is one of the largest of the mound-birds, and the only one commonly seen in captivity. OVEN-BIRD. 'The Oven-bird is brown and white, about the size of a Thrush, and has the strutting gait of a Bantam cock. WING-CLAWS FOR CLIMBING 133 use in a modern bird, if only during youth, for as the bird gets older and fledges, which it does in the usual uniform way, the claws are shed. This is curious, because in the Ostrich, which is ages away from any perching or climbing ancestor, being not only flightless but having lost not only the gripping hind toe but also the one next to it, the wing- claws are retained throughout life. The peculiarity of the young Hoatzin is not so unique as might appear, for the young of the Tpuracous (Musophagidtz), with which it used formerly to be classed, also pull themselves about their nests with their wings, as was first pointed out by Sir Harry Johnston, and confirmed by Mrs. Johnstone from observation on a specimen bred in her aviary. Young Moorhens and Porphyrios also use their downy wings (which have a claw on the first finger) in scrambling about, and young Grebes — at any rate young Dabchicks — can during the first week or so of their lives only get about in this way on land, or rather on the nest, which is all the land they know. The really most interesting point about the Hoatzin, then, would appear to be that it is apparently a link between the nestling and the chick types of young, having the imperfect clothing so common in the former and the active habits of the latter, though we are not told if it feeds itself at all. So distinct is the separation between the two types that there is only one case in which the 134 BIRD BEHAVIOUR young of one family differ in type from those of another nearly allied to it ; this being that of the young of the Sand- Grouse (Pterodidce) which are active runners, though with a very different type of down from that of young game-birds proper, it being more like true feathers. Yet the Sand-Grouse are supposed to be allied to the Pigeons, in which the young present the consummation of the help- less type, blind and pap-fed, with the down nearly always scanty, and sometimes even wanting, as in the bare black young of the Nicobar Pigeon (Calcenas nicobarica), which reminds one of a new-hatched Cormorant, Cormorants and Gannets being bare at first and downy later on. But the Plovers are also a related group, and these the Sand-Grouse approach in their aborted hind toes, in their flight and notes, and in laying spotted eggs on the bare ground, as much as they do the Pigeons in their short legs and vege- tarian habits. Those Pigeons also which have adopted a ground life and assumed a coloration strikingly like that of Sand- Grouse, nevertheless do not resemble them more than other Pigeons other- wise, so that possibly the Sand-Grouse are not so near Pigeons as some anatomists have made out. There is only one family of birds in which the young are not definitely known, and that is the extraordinary little group of Finfoots (Heliorni- thidce), of which only three species are known, all from warm climates, one African, one East Indian, and one South American. In appearance and INFANTILE UGLINESS 135 habits they are a strange blend of Rail and Cor- morant, and all we know about their young is that Prince Maximilian of Wied once shot a male of the South American species (Heliornis fulica) carrying two naked young under its wings. A male Dabchick might have been doing this, but the queer thing about the Finfoot's young is that they should have been naked, since such young always remain in the nest ; further information is certainly much to be desired, but the Asiatic bird seems everywhere rare, and nobody has taken much trouble about the African and American species, though both have been kept in captivity, one of the former having even been brought to England by Mr. J. D. Hamlyn. Besides the inactivity of passive nestlings, the absence or very slight development of down in many of them is very striking, and their extremely repulsive appearance when thus clad, or rather unclad, makes it easier to understand the descent of birds from such an unpopular class as reptiles. " The young at first are perfectly bare and very hateful," is the remark of Russ on the young of a little Finch, as quaintly translated by Dr. Butler in his book on foreign cage-birds. But there are gradations of this in the same family ; for instance, among the Passerines, the young Lyre-bird is well clothed with down, and Mr. W. Frost tells me that he finds the young of the pretty Thrush-like Pittas, at any rate the Aru Islands species, are so as well ; Canaries and Robins have a little down, but Sparrows and Crows none. 136 BIRD BEHAVIOUR Among Parrots, young Cockatiels and Keas have down, but Budgerigars and Ring-necked Parrakeets are bare. So, apparently, are all Kingfishers, Rollers, Woodpeckers, and Bee-eaters, but Hoopoes boast of hus), to say nothing of the self-yellow coat of the Lion Mar- moset (Midas rosalia) which surpasses that of any duckling. Several weasels also boast of yellow throats and breasts, and squirrels almost rival the gayest monkeys in brilliancy of tint. Moreover, young birds, like mammals, may dis- play other bright colours if these are limited to the skin, or at any rate to featherless parts. The red on the head of young Coots and Grebes may be 140 BIRD BEHAVIOUR compared to that on the Mandrill's Bardolphian nose, and the young of the Indian Paddy-bird (Ardeola grayi), a very near relative of the Squacco Heron of our British list, display among their tufts of long hairy-looking down a skin of as lively a green as any monkey can show in uncovered portions. It may happen that young birds are far prettier than their parents, as in the case of the charmingly striped and red-capped young Grebes ; in this case they remind one of reptiles, in which the young are generally very much more beautifully and distinctly marked than the old, a contrast which is especially marked in the case of crocodiles and tortoises, which are quite handsome when young, though so parti- cularly dull when adult ; and every naturalist must have noticed the pretty silver-and-black young of the plain-coloured slow- worm (Anguis fragilis), and the rich sharp tints of young snakes. According to their reptilian descent, young birds must at first have been active and variegated, and tended to degenerate into uniform down or even nakedness, as they departed further from the ancestral type. The flying, independent young of the Mound-builders, hatched as they are without incubation, ought to be primitive, but they are said to have a suit of down which they cast in the shell, and at any rate they are not striped, a fact which militates against the utility of striping for protection, since they particularly ought to need it, not having any parental care exercised on their behalf, but shifting for themselves from the first THE REAL BIRD'S-EYE VIEW 141 like young reptiles. Also, if striping is so protective, why are striped small animals, although not absolute rarities, so infinitely in a minority compared to the plain brown ones ? And why are the pestiferous skunks and zorillas, armed with their fetid secre- tions, striped like the young Emus and the Cereopsis goslings ? The fact is, we shall not know anything certain about the meaning of colouring in relation to pro- tection until our airmen have a chance to take to animal observation, and give us the real bird's-eye view, since some of the worst enemies of all small life must be the birds of prey, which are common in all countries where man has not killed them off to protect his poultry. Beasts and reptiles, of course, do their share of destruction, but the former mostly hunt by scent, and the latter depend much on seeing their prey move, when no colour is of any use. Although they present such great diversity in the habits and care of their young, as opposed to reptiles in which these are always active and inde- pendent, birds, as every one knows, are as uniform in their production of young as in their possession of plumage, since they all lay eggs, and eggs with a hard shell at that, unlike the parchment-coated eggs of most reptiles. But they greatly differ from reptiles in the great diversity of the colour of these eggs, which is of course the reason for the great popularity of birds'- egg-collecting. Nevertheless, many groups have kept up the 142 BIRD BEHAVIOUR reptilian custom of laying plain white eggs — or at all events very faintly tinted ones. Penguins, Grebes, Pigeons, Owls, Woodpeckers, Barbets, Tou- cans, Bee-eaters, Rollers, Swifts, Hornbills, King- fishers, and Humming-birds all lay white eggs, and although most of these lay their eggs in holes, a great number do not,, notably all the Humming- birds and nearly all the Pigeons, our common Blue-rock and the Stock-Dove being quite excep- tional in the family in their habit of breeding under cover. The eggs of the Duck tribe also are always light- coloured and unspotted if not actually white, though in this case it must be admitted that the birds always cover them when leaving the nest, as do some of the Game-birds, like the Partridge. On the whole it would seem that birds which lay white eggs simply do so because they are physically incapable of laying eggs of any other colour. With regard to the numerous spotted eggs, it may be freely admitted that when laid on the bare ground, as by Plovers, they are often really hard to find, and that here protective resemblance probably does come into play ; but in the case of the often bright- coloured and beautifully-variegated eggs of passerine birds, generally deposited in open nests in trees and bushes, the colour does not seem to be of any great importance, the great point being that the nest should be concealed. If such eggs were coloured to assimilate to the foliage, we might expect some green ones, but in practice there are no truly leaf-green eggs except those of GEMS IN PLAIN CASKETS 143 the Cassowaries, which are just like big unripe plums, and are of course laid on the ground. And as a matter of fact, eggs of any sort are practically never of a pure colour, either in ground or markings ; blue-greens, green-blues, brown-reds, and yellow- buffs are the usual tints, but no egg is azure-blue, scarlet-red, or gamboge-yellow, or even magenta or violet. The most richly coloured eggs are those of the Tinamous (Tinamida), which have a glaze like china and are self-coloured, exhibiting such tints as sea-green and plum-purple, though they soon fade when blown. The Tinamous themselves are one of the plainest-coloured families of birds in existence, not one species having any bright or conspicuous marking, and only a few even such little decorations as a red bill or yellow legs ; and it will be noticed that taking the class of birds as a whole, a beautiful bird never lays a beautiful egg, and vice versa. " The music of the moon sleeps in the plain eggs of the nightingale," says the poet, and the glories of the Peacock's rainbow train and the Golden Pheasant's glowing crest and blood-red breast are packed into eggs of a simple cream- colour, while in the same family the sombre Grouse and Quail lay quite richly mottled eggs. One of the most curious coincidences among birds is to be found in the markings of the eggs of many birds of prey, which are just like dried blood, foreshadowing the little murderers which will break through their shells ; a believer in ma- 144 BIRD BEHAVIOUR ternal impressions would tell us that this is just what one might expect, but the white eggs of the equally sanguinary Owls would confute him. But the existence of such meaningless coincidences should make us careful in assigning a survival value to every modification of form and colour in either bird or egg ; the subconical shape of the Guille- mot's egg, for instance, certainly renders it less likely to roll off the ledge on which it is laid, but then one parent bird or the other is always on it, and the peg-top shape of the eggs of Plovers and shore-birds, although it facilitates packing in the cross-shaped arrangement, points all inwards, which the sitting bird affects for them, may be merely incidental and not specially evolved. Sometimes there seems to be no reason whatever for the peculiarities of an egg ; why, for instance, should the curious Pink-headed Duck of India (Rbodonessa caryophyllacea) lay, in an ordinary Duck- nest, eggs which in colour, gloss, and roundness remind one of a set of billiard-balls ? Then as to markings and tint, no reason has been given why the Guillemot should lay eggs so extraordinarily variable in these respects, or why birds which lay coloured or variegated eggs should, generally speak- ing, exhibit so much difference in their output, as compared with the uniformity of their plumage. Broadly speaking, one may expect in any species laying a variegated egg specimens lightly and heavily spotted, and others with fairly evenly distributed markings contrasting with some in AN ORIGINAL GUILLEMOT 145 which the colouring tends to concentrate in a belt or cap, almost invariably at the large end — phenomena which are well exhibited in the common Sparrow. No other bird is anything like so variable in its egg-coloration as the Guillemots, but some are very far from being even ordinarily true to type, as the Red-backed Shrike with its olive and red types, some Indian Warblers with their blues and reds, and some of the African Weavers with their self-blues, spotted blues, and whites. Individual birds, however, tend to reproduce the same type of egg ; there is an historical instance of a Guillemot which laid on a particular rock-shelf an egg of the rare and valuable red variety which was duly taken for fifteen years on end — though of course this might have been the produce of another bird with the same peculiarity. Yet on the other hand, all the eggs in a laying may not match ; the Tree-Sparrow is notorious for having one differently marked egg in every set, and the Golden Eagle commonly lays one variegated egg and one plain one. It is said, also, that in the case of birds which lay richly coloured eggs, the coloration varies as the bird advances in years, which is, after all, rather what one might expect. The colour is in any case laid on almost like paint, and will smear if the egg be washed when new-laid ; while in the case of the eggs of many groups — Shore-birds (Cbaradriidtz), Gulls, Rails, and song-birds especially — it will be noted that there are often two sets of spots, one of which 10 146 BIRD BEHAVIOUR has been laid on before the shell is complete and so lies deeper and looks fainter, such under-surface spots being grey, lilac, or mauve in colour. In the eggs of Cormorants and their kin (Stegano- podes), Grebes, and some of the non-parasitic American Cuckoos (Crotophaga, Guira) there is a chalky layer external to the real shell, which is often blue, and shows here and there. In the case of the Cuckoo Guira, the "White Ani," the effect is exceedingly pretty, the rich blue shell being plenti- fully flecked, but not obscured, by the chalky secretion. Grebes' eggs, by being covered by the owners with some of the sodden weed of which the nest is made, soon get stained and are generally seen as buff or brown objects, though the proper and original colour is white ; Ducks' eggs are naturally greasy on the surface. The egg of the Fulmar is distinguishable by the nose, having the same strong smell as characterizes every part of its producer, even to one feather. The surface of birds' eggs generally varies a good deal, from a rough one like unglazed porcelain to the glazed type best shown in the Tinamous' eggs ; but Kingfishers and Owls also lay very glossy eggs, and Ostriches' eggs are both glossy and pitted. The shell gets duller and more brittle as the eggs are sat upon in the case of birds generally. The number of the sitting varies in a very inter- esting way ; in quite a number of birds it is at the minimum, the egg being single. This is the case with Petrels and Albatrosses, the Gannets, the A LOVE OF ODD NUMBERS 147 American Vultures, the Lammergeier, the Sun- Bittern, the White Noddy (Gygis), the Crab-Plover (Dromas ardeola), and many Auks, Penguins, Hum- ming-birds, and Pigeons. Most Pigeons and Hum- ming-birds, however, lay two eggs, as do some of the diving sea-fowl just mentioned. Four is the ordinary number with the shore- birds — Plovers, Sandpipers, and so forth — and it is only among these that a larger set than one or two is constant ; as a general rule when twins are exceeded, " several " is the only numerical expres- sion which can be used to describe the hatch. Birds which do not lay two or four eggs seem, however, often to think that there is luck in odd numbers, for three, five, and seven are frequent numbers. There is a widespread tendency, as that very acute observer, Mr. E. C. Stuart Baker, has pointed out, to over-rate the number of eggs laid by the Game-birds and Ducks ; these are really not so much more prolific than the birds nursing helpless young in nests as is supposed, at any rate in the East. The wild Hen oftener lays six eggs than a dozen, and this will be a reasonable brood for many Ducks ; the Australian Musk-Duck (Biziura lobata) only lays three eggs. It would appear, however, that in Europe most birds, Passerines included, are more prolific than in the Tropics. The eggs of birds which have active young are credited with being bigger than those from which passive nestlings are excluded, as well as more 148 BIRD BEHAVIOUR numerous ; and certainly the largest eggs propor- tionately to the parent come from species with this habit. The Guillemot, for instance, whose young is as active as existence on a narrow shelf of rock admits, lays an enormous egg, and this is surpassed by the Kiwi, whose egg is so large that it is difficult to see where the bird's internal organs go while it is being carried, and the male bird, which alone incubates, cannot cover it properly, but has to lie across it. The Jack-Snipe, too, accomplishes something like a record in laying four eggs so big that they average over the inch in their widest diameter, while the bird is not bigger than a Lark. The Raven, on the other hand, is remarkable, even among nestling-fosterers, for the small size of its eggs, which are hardly bigger than those of the Carrion Crow, a bird hardly more than half its size and of similar general habits. The Wren, true to its original and self-assertive character, lays a large sitting of relatively large eggs for its size. It will be noted, however, that very small birds do not lay the tiny eggs one might expect ; those of Humming-birds, for instance, though among them may naturally be found the smallest of all eggs, are not proportionately so minute as one might have supposed. There is evidently a limit to the smallness of eggs ; an experienced poultry-fancier told me not long ago that this was the obstacle which faced the bantam-breeder, for whenever an exceptionally tiny pullet was reared, she was certain to succumb HENS WITH ONE CHICKEN 149 sooner or later at the attempted laying of her first egg, the egg not being reduced proportionately to the size of the hen. Something of this sort may have operated to prevent Humming-birds becoming as small as blue-bottles — the actually smallest species, Calypte belen& of Cuba, being just about as big as a queen humble-bee, though I doubt if it is as light as Mellisuga minima, which used, as its name implies, to be considered the smallest of birds. The Storm-Petrel, smallest of web-footed fowl, certainly lays a very large egg for its size, although the young is helpless ; it is not larger than a Swal- low, while its egg measures an inch in length, and this is not purely a matter of disproportion to reduced size, for Petrels generally lay a large egg. It is worth noticing that it is among birds which lay a single egg that the most sensationally numerous species are found ; the Passenger Pigeon of America (Ec topis tes migratorius), so recently extinct, whose columns used to take hours to pass a given spot, was one of the Pigeons which lay but one egg. The Pufrln is supposed by some to be the most numerous bird in Europe, and some of its brother Auks, like the Guillemot, are also inordinately numerous ; the extinct Great Auk was wonderfully abundant in its day, as is the common Gannet still for a bird of its size, while according to Darwin, the Fulmar Petrel is said to be the most numerous bird in the world. This is very doubtful, but he might have been thinking of another Petrel, the Mutton-bird (Puffinus brevicauda), which has been seen in enor- ISO BIRD BEHAVIOUR mous masses in the South Pacific, quite equalling the old-time flocks of Passenger Pigeons. At any rate, such facts as these look as if one- egg-laying were a good habit for survival ; but at the same time we must remember that the birds above quoted are conspicuous for living or at any rate breeding, in masses, thus giving a false impres- sion of their numbers as compared to more scattered species ; that most of them draw their living from the hospitable storehouse of the sea, and that two of them did not stand long against human destruc- tiveness at any rate, and might not have withstood the invasion of any other carnivore. Moreover, some of the single- egg-layers are scarce birds ; the Condor and Lammergeier are not nearly so common as the smaller Vultures — I only saw two of the latter during all my residence in India ; and the large Hornbills, which lay only one egg, are always rare where the smaller and more prolific kinds are common. There may be in some cases good reason why only one egg should be laid, as will appear in the consideration of some curious incubation and nursing habits, but it is a quite possible consideration that birds which lay only one egg are on the decline as species through infertility, since one egg a year — and sometimes even one in two years, as is said to be the case with some of the great Vultures — is a perilously low output, and the sea-birds are liable to numerous accidents. And as to actual numbers, it is highly probable that such birds as the Quail and Skylark, which are FERTILITY IN CAPTIVITY 151 common almost all across the Old World, are really more abundant than any bird which ever nested in close-packed colonies and flew in cloud-like flocks. The fertility of birds laying several eggs appears to increase with age ; the Swan, for instance, lays twice as many eggs when mature as she usually does when starting laying in her second year. It is true that domestic Hens decline in fertility in a year or two ; but then they are expected to lay in one year twenty times as many as their wild ancestress does. Tame Ducks are more fertile ; with an equally exaggerated egg-production, they lay a paying amount of eggs — at any rate in the case of the prolific Indian Runner breed — for twice as many years as a hen. Yet the Muscovy Duck, the Turkey, and the Goose have not had their fertility increased as a rule above that of wild birds. These always have eggs in reserve ; even if the set be only one or two, others will replace it at least once in case of accident, and birds which lay several eggs will under artificial stimulation produce numbers at times as great as an inferior domestic Hen. An unfeeling naturalist once made a Wryneck lay forty eggs in a season by removing them as fast as laid, a piece of brutality which very probably ended in sterilizing its subject, as an extreme output in one year is likely to reduce subsequent prolificacy, as one would expect. In a case I heard of in which a hen Silver Pheasant laid seventy eggs in one season, iS2 BIRD BEHAVIOUR I was afterwards told that she had laid none at all the next. What may be called voluntary overlaying is rather apt, however, to occur in captive birds ; the Californian Quail (Lo^hortyx californicus) is particularly liable to it. It may even occur in Nature ; thus, a specimen of the Indian Nukta or Comb-Duck (Sarcidiornis melanonota) has been captured on a regular deposit of eggs, tier after tier, amounting to forty in number; and as she was very emaciated, and one of the eggs was very small, presumably her last, she had no doubt laid the lot. Such cases as these, and exceptional Turkey hens that lay a hundred eggs, show us how our high- laying races of Fowls and Ducks have been origin- ated ; they began a long time ago, since Aristotle speaks of every-day layers. Birds may deposit their batch of eggs at the rate of one egg a day, or lay at intervals of two or more days ; the rule is not constant for the same group, since the Pigeon deposits her second egg two days after laying" her first, and the Collared Dove lays her pair on con- secutive days. With hens and Turkeys, too, every- day or intermittent laying varies individually. Most birds, although keeping about the nest, do not begin to sit till they have got the full number ; the rule appears to be universal among those which have active young, since their broods all come off at once, but birds may make a mistake or get impatient, and go off with a partial brood, leaving THE FATE OF BENJAMIN 153 tardy or insufficiently incubated hatchlings to perish, in the shell. Many birds which have nestling young, however, evidently begin to sit at once, since nestlings of very different ages may be found in the same nest ; this is notoriously the case with some Owls — such as the Barn-Owl — and Hawks, and it is noticeable with Budgerigars, at any rate in captivity. In such a case, the youngest member of the brood may be callow and blind, while the eldest is feathered and almost ready to fly ; and in such cases it is curious that the youngest can survive at all, espe- cially in the case of birds of prey, where it must offer considerable temptation to the appetites of its nest-fellows. Indeed, a case has been recorded (" Notes on Cage-birds," ed. Greene, 2nd series) in which a brood of young Barn-Owls when shut up in a room with a supply of meat nevertheless killed and ate the Benjamin of the family, and a brood of Canaries I reared myself all hatched out on different days, the five eggs having been laid, as usual with such birds, consecutively, with the result that the youngest, which was a little overdue and helped out by me with a pin, was fatally crushed, with the next youngest, by the other three. To avoid such occurrences, it is the custom of many canary- fanciers to take away the eggs as fast as they are laid, replacing them with a nest-egg, and putting them back when the complement is complete ; but this is not always done, and the wild small birds 154 BIRD BEHAVIOUR generally get on successfully with their broods, no doubt not being tempted to sit so closely as Canaries, owing to the need of seeking food for themselves. I noticed with the Dabchicks which bred on the Indian Museum pond that they seemed to attach no value whatever to their eggs till they had the full set ; the first laid were allowed to lie, con- spicuous as they were by their whiteness, uncovered in the nest, which was only among the low-growing kalmi or water- convolvulus or among thin reeds. I can only conclude that the eggs escaped the attention of the numerous House-Crows simply by the dread these worthies had of risking their pre- cious persons in the attempt to pick up an object lying practically on the surface of the water, for I found they did not dare to pick up bread thrown into the pond, although the same species of Crow down by the Hooghly picked food from the water as a matter of course. When the Dabchicks began incubating, however, they were careful enough, and always covered the eggs before leaving them, after the usual manner of Grebes ; curiously enough, however, though they kept the nest piled up while they were brooding the young on it, they did not raise it when they had eggs and these were menaced by a flood, and lost two sittings in this way in consequence. Under similar circumstances a Swan, as is well known, will raise her nest, but then she is always ready to do this, if material is within reach of her bill. I noticed that with these Dabchicks incubation KEEPING THE EGGS COOL 155 was very largely left to the sun, or to the heat generated by the decaying weed in which the eggs lay, for they were often both off the nest at all and any times of the day ; and this habit of hatch- ing otherwise than by constant incubation has been often noticed. The Black-back Courser (Pluvianus agyptius) of the African river-banks buries its eggs in the sand and damps them to prevent them getting too hot ; Sand-Grouse on the plains of India have been found sitting on the eggs not to warm them but to keep them cool, since under the blazing sun they actually began to cook if the birds were scared off for a little time ; and the Ostrich has quite a reputation for letting the sun hatch its eggs, though this only happens in the more tropical part of its range — in South Africa it sits like any other bird. Ordinary passerine birds may even in exceptional circumstances make use of the sun as an incubating . agent ; in Hume's " Nests and Eggs of Indian Birds " there is a record of a pair of House-Mynahs (Acridotberes tristis) which built in a hole under the edge of a roof fully exposed to the Indian hot- weather sun, with the result that both of the pair were always to be seen off the nest, the cock singing vociferously and thrashing casual Crows after the usual manner of his kind. There is a whole family of birds, allied to the Game-birds, which carry matters further, and never sit on their eggs under any circumstances, these being the Megapodes or Mound-builders, referred 156 BIRD BEHAVIOUR to above as producing young which are able to shift for themselves as soon as hatched. Owing to one of the largest of them, the Australian Brush- Turkey (Cathe 'turns lath ami), being frequently im- ported, their habits in at least one case are fairly well known, and have besides been studied in their native haunts in the case of several other species. Generally speaking, they live in Australia and New Guinea, but one, the Maleo (Megacepbalon maleo), is found in Celebes, and the birds of the typical genus Megapodius, which are more like large dull Partridges than anything else, are widely spread from the Nicobars to well out in the Pacific Islands — Ninafou and Samoa. All lay very large eggs for their size, so that here at any rate large size of egg corresponds with high development of the young when hatched, and make up their natural incubators with their powerful feet, in which the hind toe is well developed and low set, although they do not perch more than the typical Game-birds. Their claws are often very long and strong, and they present the peculiarity, only found elsewhere among birds in the Cuckoos, of having the second and third toes, i.e. the inner and middle front ones, joined by a short web at the base, whereas this web, where it exists in other birds, is either between the outer and middle toes — third and fourth — as in the Herons, or connects all the toes, as in typical Game-birds. I mention this point because it is commonly supposed that small points not obviously connected with habits, and hence of value in classi- NATURAL INCUBATORS 157 fication, are only to be discovered in bird anatomy, whereas they occur externally also. The Mound-birds always lay several eggs, and always bury them ; but their procedure in this respect varies : the Maleo simply inters them in beaches of black volcanic sand, and is supposed to deposit them in the hatching-holes at intervals of a fortnight. The Brush-Turkey hens, however, lay every two days, and the species of this family generally scratch up mounds of earth, sand, and dead vegetable matter, that of the Brush-Turkey having a circumference of three dozen yards, with a height of a little over two feet. In this species, as observed in captivity at all events, the male does all the work of preparing the mound and looking after the eggs, when laid and buried, driving the hen or hens off as soon as they have laid, and then covering them deeply or thinly as occasion seems to demand. As many as twelve to fifteen eggs may be laid by one of these birds, but sometimes several pairs may share a mound, though this seems not to be the case with the typical Megapodii, which also use more sand and less vegetable matter than the Brush-Turkey. The latter bird breeds in the preserves of the Duke of Bedford, and did so in the Zoo in a quite small enclosure during the Bartlett period, and of late years when Mr. Bertling was head-keeper. When hatched, the young scratch their way out or are dug out by the old birds ; and not only are they hardy when hatched, but their development 158 BIRD BEHAVIOUR in the egg appears to be able to proceed in some cases without any extraneous heat, such as is cer- tainly generated in the Brush-Turkey's mound by the decaying vegetable matter. More than a dozen years ago, some eggs of the Nicobar Megapode were taken from a mound in the islands and put on board the Government ship Elphinstone. Here they were allowed to lie about on deck in a bucket, and when the ship got up to the British settlement in the Andamans they were put away in a cup- board ; nevertheless some chicks at all events hatched out and were reared, and I saw these at the Calcutta Zoo, where they were sent when full grown. It would appear, however, that the tem- perature in these Megapodes' mounds is not high, the part where the eggs are buried being if anything cool to the touch. The Mound-birds are a group with a long incu- bation-period, the Brush-Turkey, a bird of the common Fowl's size, taking six weeks ; the incuba- tion-period of birds being a group-character or minor habit, differing in length according to the family to which the bird belongs, but, in the case of birds of the same family, being generally in proportion to the size of the species, large birds generally taking a longer time than small ones. Thus, among our familiar tame birds, the Fowl, as most people know, takes three weeks, the Turkey and Peacock a month ; the common Duck takes a month, the large Muscovy Duck five weeks, the Goose four, the Swan six. Both the Duck and SIZE AND SITTING-TIME 159 Fowl belong to long-period families ; among the short-period birds we have in domestication, the Pigeon takes seventeen and the Collared Dove fifteen days, and the Canary a fortnight, both Pigeons and Passerines having short incubation periods. Owing to this family limitation of periods, we get some very curious contrasts ; such big birds as the Crowned Pigeon (Goura coronata) and the Raven only taking three weeks, while the tiny Painted Quail (Excalfactoria sinensis), no bigger than a Sparrow, and the Teal, not larger than a Pigeon, take as long, and the Storm-Petrel even five weeks, in spite of its small size. It will be seen that however small the bird, the reduction of its incubation-period does not keep pace with the size ; nor does the increase in the case of large species. Some of the smallest Finches only sit for eleven days, and even the Humming-birds are only credited with a day less ; the longest incuba- tion period is that of the Emu, in which the devoted male sits for nine weeks, longer than the Ostrich, with its six or seven, but this bird belongs to a distinct family, all the clans of flightless giants being very well differentiated anatomically, and evidently representing independent degenerations from flying ancestors, though of an early type. In a few cases the smaller bird in the same family may take a longer time than a larger ; thus Part- ridges and Gold Pheasants take longer to hatch than Fowl chicks. The case of the common Pheasant, 160 BIRD BEHAVIOUR which also takes longer, need not be mentioned, because the wild fowl is not so large as this bird, and curiously enough increase of size in domestica- tion, however great, does not seem to affect the incubation period to any noticeable extent. There does not seem to be any general correlation between the activity of the chick and the period of incubation ; the young of the H£mipodes or Button- Quails, birds about the size of Quails proper, hatch in only twelve days, although downy and active, while the helpless nestlings of the Budgerigar, an even smaller bird, take nearly three weeks, Parrots being a long-period group ; and the nestlings of birds of prey require a long time to hatch, the largest, the Condor, even requiring six weeks. On the whole there is a tendency for water-fowl to take a longer time than land-birds, no aquatic groups having a really short period, though none take so long as some of the great runners. The period of incuba- tion does not vary more than a day or two unless over a month, when the variation may be a matter of several days, as in the case of the Swan and Ostrich. As the temperatures of different groups vary, incubation may be hastened artificially in some cases ; when Mandarin Ducks were first bred at the Zoo, now nearly seventy years ago, it was noticed that those of their eggs which were put under hens hatched two days earlier than those which the Ducks themselves were allowed to sit upon. M. Rogeron, in discussing Hens as foster- TESTING THE TIME-LIMIT 161 parents for these and other fancy ducklings, con- demns some Hens as altogether too hot, so that the eggs entrusted to them come out so much too soon that the young are weakly and unsettled, and die off through sheer inability to live. The frequency with which eggs of birds have been placed in domestic conditions under other individuals or species has brought out conspicuously the great difference in patience in sitting birds ; Hens and Turkeys will often sit out two periods if compelled or even allowed, while Pigeons refuse to incubate more than a day or two over their limit, and I do not remember any case of water- fowl exceeding their time. However, the Hooded Crow, although one would have thought this cunning bird not easily duped, will, according to Graham in his " Birds of lona and Mull," sit so far over her time when her eggs have been removed, boiled, and replaced by boys, that she is easily captured through the weakness and exhaustion caused by this unnaturally protracted incubation. Evidently the Crows are not immune from the extreme mental upset which occurs during the incubation period ; poultry-keepers well know that the most effectual cure for a broody Hen is to place her in a coop with barred floor and supported on legs, for some Hens will sit almost indefinitely, not only on any object they can imagine to be an egg, but even on an empty nest. By making the bird continually perch, however,- the delusion becomes impossible of maintenance, especially if ii 162 BIRD BEHAVIOUR her mind is distracted by seeing the other Fowls enjoying life outside her prison. The domestic Hen is not alone in being able to solace her mind with a substitute for an egg ; Hume in his " Nests and Eggs of Indian Birds " gives an amusing account of a Kite which sat upon a pill-box until rain wrecked this treasure ; and a pair of Cranes nesting in captivity, but unable to produce eggs, fished up a couple of pieces of brick from the bottom of their pond and made believe they were eggs. Facts like these are of importance when we consider the ease with which birds, even of intelligent kinds, may be duped into caring for eggs of parasitic species. There is, as a matter of fact, a physical as well as mental change in a sitting bird ; the abdomen becomes inflamed and bare, constituting the " hatch- ing spot," and no doubt it is irritation here, not a self-sacrificing feeling, which makes Ducks pull off the down of their breasts which they then use as a nest-lining. In the hole-building species indeed this practically constitutes all the nest, but in tropical Ducks, although these often, in fact per- haps usually, breed in holes of trees, this lining is scanty, and may be wanting altogether. The commercial importance of the female Eider's down — the drake contributes none, apparently even in an emergency — is well known, but a physio- logical fact about this bird which is less familiar is its power of enduring a fast while sitting, at any rate in captivity, Mr. St. Quintin, one of the few SUPPRESSION OF SCENT 163 people who have bred the species, recording that a bird of his seemed never to leave the nest during the whole time of sitting, so that chickweed growing around grew all over its back. In any case birds need little food while sitting, and their excreta are concentrated in a remarkable way ; any one who keeps Fowls or Pigeons has noticed that when a sitting hen leaves the nest she voids a large mass all at once, and the scent appears herein to be concentrated, thus explaining the fact that the body-scent of a sitting game- bird, so perceptible at other times to the nose of a questing beast of prey, becomes suppressed, being as it were driven inwards. In spite of the wearisome nature of the task as it appears to u^, it is evident that birds must experience great satisfaction in the action of incubation, as also in the more toilsome but seemingly more interesting process of rearing the young, since they are so irresistibly impelled to it that in many cases it is repeated several times during the year ; in fact, some tame birds, such as fancy Pigeons, Canaries, and Budgerigars, would endanger their health and produce weakly offspring by continually breeding — in the case of the last even in out- door aviaries in mid- winter — if their owners did not by removing nesting facilities, or even by separating the sexes, put a stop to propaga- tion after a reasonable number of broods had been raised. Birds which have active young are less philo- 1 64 BIRD BEHAVIOUR progenitively inclined, the rearing of one brood satisfying as a rule both wild and domestic species of this character for the year, even the pampered park Mallard not breeding in autumn, though they pair ; and among the birds which have helpless young, or at any rate young which have to be fed, the least fertile, i.e. those which lay but one egg at a time, are also the least apt to want to repeat the process. The diving rock-birds which throng our cliffs, for instance, depart to sea quite content with their single chick. Pigeons, however, are quite willing and anxious to repeat their task of rearing twins as often as occasion allows, that is to say, as long as they can get plenty of food. I have seen Wood-Pigeons in Regent's Park, where conditions are exceptionally favourable, pairing at Christmas in very cold weather, and in India nests of the commonest species of Turtle-Doves are to be found practically all the year round, so that these birds apparently are always ready to breed, except, no doubt, when moulting, at which time the most enthusiastic nesters have perforce to postpone operations. Ordinary passerine birds, also, frequently rear two or even more broods in a season, so that even when their sittings are individually less in number than those of birds with active chicks, their total output for the year is larger. There are also certain individual species which have a special bent for procreation, and will commence nesting on less provocation, if I may so express it, than any others ; POPULAR RECORD-BREAKERS 165 such are the birds that are commended as " good breeders" by aviarists, and in wild nature in this country we may notice that it is the Song- Thrush and the Robin which generally contribute the " early bird's nest " newspaper records, leaving even such prolific birds as the Blackbird and the Sparrow far behind in the race for actual priority of com- mencement, though these may equal the output before the year is over. Generally speaking, however, it may be said that wild birds nest only once a year, and some of the large birds of prey are said only to do so in alternate years. CHAPTER VI Propagation (continued) — Nest-making not purely a bird-habit — Eggs laid without nests — Types of nests — Parasitic nesting — Parasitic layers, like Cuckoos and Cow-birds — Degreci of development of parasitic instinct. BIRDS are not alone in incubating their eggs any more than in laying eggs at all ; for pythons among snakes incubate their eggs, coiling round them and undergoing a rise of temperature, while the Echidna of Australia, one of the only two egg-laying mammalian types — the other oviparous beast being the Duck-billed Platypus — carries her single egg in a temporarily formed pouch. The two eggs of the Platypus are laid in her burrow. Neither is nest-making confined to birds, for, putting aside the extraordinary structure made by many insects, we have nest-makers among the vertebrates in the persons of many fish— including our little stickleback — in some tropical frogs, and in numerous beasts from the gorilla to the harvest- mouse. These points are worth mentioning, be- cause some naturalists theorize about birds' nests as if nest-making were a special bird habit, whereas for all we know the particular reptile which pre- ceded the first definite bird may have been a nest- builder already. 166 INCUBATION WHILE STANDING 167 Be that as it may, many existing birds make nests no more elaborate than those of some reptiles ; the practice of the Mound-builders in burying their eggs is much the same as that followed by crocodiles and tortoises, and the African or common crocodile (Crocodilus niloticus) has been observed in Mada- gascar sleeping on her deposit of eggs, and is be- lieved to keep on hand to dig up the young when they hatch, and take them off. This is very like the action of the Mound-birds — and also of those fish which guard the nest, aerate the eggs, and pro- tect the young — and as the crocodiles are the nearest related of present-day reptiles to birds, as their anatomy and bird-like eggs testify, it is tempting to regard the Megapodes as the most primitive of nesters, especially considering the advanced state of their young on exclusion from the egg. If, however, a previous coat of down is shed in the shell, they are not so primitive as they look, probably, but have gone back to the simple life. In any case, there are birds which do far less in the way of nesting. The two giant Penguins, the King (ApUnodytes -pennanti) and the Emperor (A. forsteri), for instance, do not put their single egg down anywhere, but keep it pressed up against the skin of the abdomen, which bulges over it, and supported by the feet, which are kept close together, and so incubate standing. The bird thus cannot walk freely, but it can shuffle about awkwardly, and thus move its egg from place to place if necessary. The habit seems reasonable enough in the Emperor, 1 68 BIRD BEHAVIOUR which breeds on the Antarctic ice at mid-winter, of all seaaons, and so needs to protect the egg and chick from cold before everything, but in the King, which does not reach the Antarctic circle at any time, and breeds on land, the habit seems less significant. It is curious, too, that as the great danger of the infant Emperor's life is the scrimmage for its pos- session which results when it is seen, all childless Emperors desiring to foster it, the only break in the whiteness of its down is the black skull-cap, making its little head conspicuous against the white belly of the parent, and thus proclaiming the possession of the treasure. The young King is quite different, being dark brown all over, though the parents are so much alike. It is easy to see why these birds only lay a single egg, and also why this is the case with the Guillemots and Razorbill, and with the White Noddies, which have the next simplest nidification, simply laying the egg on an unprepared surface and keeping constantly on it. The nesting-places of the Auks above mentioned are high cliff-ledges, often even sloping seawards, so that the egg, in spite of its sub-conical shape, would roll off if one or other of the birds did not constantly sit on it, which they do facing the rock, no doubt so as to minimize the chance of dragging the egg off with them when leaving it. But that of the tropical White Noddies, re- nowned for their spirit-like appearance and uncanny tameness, is not only a point of a coral reef, but PHENOMENAL FORESIGHT 169 equally or more often the branch of a tree or the broad leaf of a palm ; any slight depression is chosen if possible, but often even this is dispensed with. The bird covers and leaves its egg very gingerly, and is credited with such foresight as, when laying on a leaf, so to time the hatching that if the leaf withers and droops, by the time the slope has become too dangerous, the egg will have hatched. The chick has great powers of holding on, having particularly long middle claws ; the hind toe is also of normal size, not rudimentary as in other Terns, and the feet are only half-webbed. As the plumage is also scanty for a water-bird, and the bird nests inland and even eats fruit, we have here perhaps a primitive Gull-type. Other birds that simply lay their eggs on the spot chosen without any nest are the Nightjars proper, though the Owlet- Night jars lay in holes in trees, and the Frog-mouths, members of the same family as the last (Podargida) make nests, of sticks in the case of the well-known Australian More-pork (Podargus cuvieri) and of a curious pad of down on a branch in the case of the Eared Frog- mouths (Batrachostomus). Simple deposition of the eggs on the ground is unusual in birds of the type to which Nightjars belong, but is not uncommon among running or swimming birds; Divers, Sand-Grouse, Bustards, and many of the Game-birds, Plovers and Terns making either no nest at all, a mere " scrape," or a very meagre collection of bents, leaves, etc. The habit 1 70 BIRD BEHAVIOUR varies, for in the same ternery some nests will have practically no furnishing, and others be fairly well provided. Some birds, such as the Ringed Plover when breeding inland, and the Adelie Penguin of the Antarctic (Pygoscelis adelite) build nests of stones, which in the latter case are indeed the only available material ; and the male Penguin " proposes " by offering the hen a pebble. In the case of this Penguin the nesting-site appears to belong to the hen, which arrives first and takes possession ; but in some cases at all events nesting-site property is vested in the male bird, which brings his hen to his chosen haunt, or arrives there first and guards it for her, when the two have been separated on migration, as male birds generally arrive first. The next simplest plan to laying the eggs on the ground is laying them in a hole therein, or in a rock or tree, without any nest ; this is a common practice, especially characteristic of Parrots, Owls, and some other non-passerine perching birds, such as Toucans and Hornbills. Holes often have to be cleaned out, and so this practice passes naturally into the widely-spread custom of the bird excavating its burrow itself; in fact, the two habits are hard to separate, as often the same species will occupy a hole if it is there already, or make one for itself if such accommodation is wanting. Burrowing is found in scattered members of many groups, such as the Burrowing Owl (Speotyto cunicularia) of America, the Sand-Martin and some CONCAVE-CASQUED HORNBILL. This Asiatic species is shown with the nesting female characteristically walled up in a hole in a tree. 170 BIRDS AS BURROWERS 171 other Swallows, the Minera (Geositta cunicularia), a South American passerine bird, some Penguins, and the Puffin amongst the Auks ; but it is espe- cially characteristic of birds which sally forth for their prey from a perch and return to it, using their feet but little, such as Kingfishers, Bee-eaters, Todies, and Motmots, and of Petrels among the sea-birds. Bee- eaters are most inveterate burrowers, forming regular warrens and even sometimes burrowing into level ground as Petrels do, most bird-burrowers generally liking a bank for their tunnels. It has been said that animals which do not get their living by burrowing do not show any special adaptation for it, and the Sand-Martin is a case in point ; but the Kingfishers and other burrowing perchers, where burrowing is a family habit, are noteworthy for the union of their toes, forming a flat broad sole, which must be of some use in throw- ing out the sand. It is true, however, that Horn- bills have similar feet, although they do not burrow, but nest in holes in trees as said above ; but they are near akin to the other joined- toed families. Kingfishers when starting a burrow have been seen to dart at the bank and dislodge the earth by this charge, the pair relieving each other and repeating the action until a big enough depression was made to allow them to get to work with their feet as well. Parrots with their gnawing beaks and powerful short legs are better adapted for burrowing than any other birds, and some do thus nest in banks, such as the Patagonian Conure (Cyano- 1 72 BIRD BEHAVIOUR lyseus fatagonus), one of the few Parrots living in temperate and cold climates, as a usual thing, and the well-known Lemon-crested and Rose-breasted Cockatoos occasionally. The strangest of burrowers is, however, the remarkable pied Crab-Plover (Dromas ardeola) of the shores of Eastern seas, for one would never expect a long-legged wader like this to burrow ; yet it does so, and lays at the end of its bow-shaped tunnel one large white egg, having in fact the nesting- habits of a Petrel. This bird is a primitive type, being curiously intermediate in appearance between a Plover and a Gull, with half- webbed feet provided with a well- developed hind toe, unlike the families to which it is most nearly related. As to what long-legged birds can do in the way of digging, I never realized this till I saw the cock Rhea making his " Scrape," which he does in a crouching position, and nevertheless thus uses his powerful limbs to great effect. Among the Woodpeckers, the Ground Wood- pecker of the Cape (Geocolaptes olivaceus) burrows in banks, but, as every naturalist knows, these birds generally cut out their nesting-holes in trees ; this is all part of the day's work for the Woodpecker, which is a carpenter by trade, but the Barbets do it too, although not pecking wood otherwise, and curiously enough have a fancy for beginning the hole from the underside of the bough, so that they have to commence operations upside down. Where burrowing or tree-hole-cutting is a family AN ABNORMAL KINGFISHER 173 habit, no nest is made by the bird, the eggs being laid on the floor of the hole ; in fact, such birds throw out all they find inside a hole, and the Starling greatly annoys and even dispossesses the much larger and more powerful Green Woodpecker by carrying nesting-stuff into his borings. When, however, the hole-builder, burrower or merely appropriator, is a member of a family whose general custom is to nest outside, something like the normal nest of the family, though often a very slovenly one, is made, as by the Sand-Martin, Jackdaw, Stock-Dove, and Sheldrake, and the many kinds of Tits. This points to the hole-building in these cases being a recent habit, but some members of genuine troglodyte families may as an abnormality build a nest ; Mr. E. C. Stuart Baker has found the Indian White-breasted Kingfisher (Halcyon smyr- nensis) doing so, the structure being a domed one of moss. There is some connection between making a domed nest and breeding in holes, for, as every one knows, the House-Sparrow, whose nest is naturally a domed one in a tree, readily takes to nesting in holes, and so do many of the small foreign Finches commonly imported. In the case of the Tree- Sparrow, the hole- building habit has apparently nearly superseded the tree- nesting one, a nest of this species anywhere but in a hole — of a tree in most places in Europe, or of a building in the Himalayas, Japan, and a few other places — being cited as a notable exception. This is 174 BIRD BEHAVIOUR of interest, because the Tree-Sparrow is in some respects the most advanced species of the true Sparrows, having in both sexes a coloration which is masculine only in its nearest allies, and the widest distribution, for the House-Sparrow, though actually found in more countries, has in many cases been transported deliberately by man, whereas its rival colonizes on its own account. Nesting with a covering of some sort, found existing or constructed, overhead, is a habit of which birds which possess it are very tenacious, and however intelligent they may be in other ways, such birds seldom venture to nest in the open ; of this the Sparrow, Starling, and Sheldrake are conspicuous examples. Nevertheless among birds which make nests as opposed to burrowers, dome- builders are the exception, most constructed nests being open, either mere platforms or piles, such as those of the Woodpigeon and the Moorhen, or advancing to the state of a more or less deep cup, such as is built by most passerine birds. Such nests with elevated sides or domed roofs may be built by a process of felting, as when made with moss and similar substances, or actually woven with a skill which is at times most admirable. Unfortunately we are not well off in the north for very skilful nest-builders, and the only woven nest of a British-breeding bird is that of the Golden Oriole, a species unfortunately too much persecuted to have other than the rarest chances of exhibiting its skill ; the nest is a woven hammock, suspended TROUPIALS AND ORIOLES 175 in the fork of a bough, and this is the type of nest built by the true Orioles generally. The Troupials, wrongly called Orioles in America, from their frequently similar yellow- and- black coloration and general habits, build, however, far more wonderful nests, though curiously enough these are woven also ; their constructions are long bags like the old-fashioned netted purses, with the entrance hole at the top near the point of suspension to the bough. Woven pendent nests, but with the entrance at the opposite end, are also made by the Weaver-birds, whose stronghold is in Africa, though the Baya (Ploceus baya), one of the best builders, inhabits South-west Asia. The males in these birds also are commonly yellow and black, forming a curious but possibly significant correlation of a type of colouring elsewhere very unusual in birds with the supremest of skill in nest-building. In the nest of the Bayas proper (Ploceus baya and P. atrigula), which are about as perfect as any of these Weavers in building ability, we get first a suspension cord attached to the end of the thin bough or section of a palm-frond which bears the nest ; then the bulb of the nest itself, which at one side passes downwards into the long entrance- spout. A section of the interior would show that the nest- cup is divided from the entrance-spout by a firmly bound partition, which prevents the young falling out, and is also used by them as a perch when older. When the nest is about half-finished — i.e. all but the cup and spout, the top half being done — 176 BIRD BEHAVIOUR this partition, then a mere bar dividing two holes, like the handle of an inverted basket, forms a useful perch for the bird working inside, a position which is taken up by the hen, which passes out the ends of the material as they are thrust in to her by the cock. It is probably owing to the want of this feminine assistance that the nest is so often left unfinished in captivity, and that so many half-built nests are found in the wild colonies, the building of these being the amusement of the males when unoccupied. Many of these will also go on lengthening the tubular entrance after the sitting has begun ; it will be noticed that this is always left loose and unbound at the end, such apparent negligence being an additional safeguard against attacks by snakes and other enemies. Another safeguarding instinct noticeable in these birds is that of hanging the nest whenever possible over water ; it has been found that even the water in the bathing-pan in an aviary incites some Weavers at all events to nest directly over this. The Baya itself is not commonly imported com- pared with some of the African Weavers, far the commonest bird of the kind in the trade being the small Red- billed Weaver of Africa (Quelea quelea), which anybody can buy cheaply and watch in any bird-room or back-garden aviary. Its nest, however, like that of many other Weavers, cannot be compared with that of the Baya, as it lacks both the suspension rope, being merely woven in amongst RESOURCEFUL CRAFTSMEN 177 twigs, and the downwardly elongated entrance- spout, being simply a round nest with, an entrance- hole below. It is, however, the skill in working the material that is the great attraction in Weavers' nests, not the exact form assumed, which is not more wonderful than that of many other nests. The Baya may at times exhibit what looks more like reason than instinct in getting its material ; cocoanut fibre or ordinary grass will do, but it has been found building with grass which was far too broad ; and in this case the bird alighted on the great blades, bit down to a sufficiently practical breadth, then went further down the blade till it had the right length, bit into the blade again, and then flew off, tearing away the strip as it went. As this grass also had a serrated edge, so that it could only be passed through one way, it is difficult to refuse reasoning powers to this particular colony of Weavers at all events. In the Weaver the constructive instinct is very much stronger in the male than in the female, but, though many birds share the labour of nest-building xwith their mates, the constructive instinct is com- monly more feminine than masculine, and some birds leave the construction entirely to the hen. The male, however, often supplies material, as generally with Sparrows and Pigeons, or he may accompany the female to and fro in her trips to get it, no doubt to keep watch over her safety while thus pre-occupied. He always takes some interest if he cares about the nest at all, which 12 178 BIRD BEHAVIOUR many males, especially among the Game-birds, do not. The male Globose Curassow (Craoc globicera), however, seems to be the chief constructor himself. Nests of the felted type, well exemplified by that of our Chaffinch in a cup form, are often built in a pensile and domed pattern as well as woven ones, especially in the tropics, where the abundance and variety of enemies render necessary in the case of all defenceless species either inaccessibility of the nest or its very perfect concealment. In India, for instance, nearly the only nests one ever ordinarily sees besides those of Crows, Kites, and other strong birds are the pensile ones of the Bayas. Now and then one may find the pensile felted nest of a Sun-bird, but this is often well concealed by looking like a bunch of rubbish ; indeed, it is made of all sorts of odds and ends — bits of dry leaf, masses of caterpillar " frass," and so forth, matted together with spiders' webs, a great stand-by for birds which build these pensile felted nests. I have even seen a pair of the Amethyst-rumped Sun-bird (Cinnyris zeylonica), the commonest kind in Calcutta, come into my verandah to carry off scraps of the grey fluff which is swept out of rooms, for use as nesting material. Extremely beautiful felted nests are built on the pensile principle by the tiny Flower-peckers (Dic&ida) of the Eastern Tropics, minute birds haunting tree-tops. These, nests are matted together with cobweb, and are so well made that FELTING AND TAILORING 179 they can be folded up without injury ; the entrance is near the top, and they look more like little wash- leather purses than anything one could imagine a bird could make — in fact, with the exception of some mud-nests presently to be described, they are the most artificial-looking products of any form of bird industry. The felted erect oval nest of our Long- tailed Tit has been long and deservedly admired, but that of the Cape Tit (JEgithalus capensis) is even more remarkable, composed as it is completely of plant- down, so closely felted that it is like cloth, and provided with a tubular entrance, not at the bottom, as in Weavers' nests, but near the top. Stark observed the hen closed this by pinching its edges together when she left the nest, either to keep the latter warm or to keep foes out, which no doubt it would do, for at any rate he once saw the mistress of the little house herself fail to get in again easily. Under this vestibule is a little pocket, in which the cock is supposed to sleep, and very likely does, Tits generally sleeping in some hole, or at any rate not on a perch in the usual way. Besides weaving and the simpler process of felting, birds occasionally practise sewing, though this form of industry is rare. The best-known sewn nest is that of the little olive-green Wren-like Warbler called from this habit the Tailor-bird, and one of the most common — and noisiest — inhabitants of Indian gardens. The nest itself is simply the i8o BIRD BEHAVIOUR lining of a cup made by sewing two or more leaves together, or the edges of one large one, and is very hard to find. The bill is of course the needle, and the thread is the raw silk of caterpillar-cocoons, which holds by reason of the resistance of this stuff to the edges of the pierced leaf, for the story that the bird makes a knot is a slight exaggeration, just like the tale of its picking up a dead leaf and sewing it to a live one. Such leaves may be found sewn together, but this is when one leaf has withered after being sewn. Several other small Warblers make sewn nests in India, and so do some of the large dull-coloured Sunbirds of the genus Arachnothera, known as Spider-hunters ; in this case the nest is secured to a broad leaf by having part of the material pushed through holes bored in this, but the structure is not nearly so perfect as the little Tailor's. So many birds mix the basket-work of their nest with mud — even the Bay a putting in a few patches — that complete nests of primitive earthenware are not surprising, though some fibre is generally, if not always, used. Such nests are familiar to us in the case of. the House-Martin and Swallow, the former being far the more perfect type. For these the birds simply take up the mud in mouthfuls, and judiciously build little at a time, lest they should overweight the foundations, as White long ago observed in his admirable chapters on these birds. Some Indian and Australian Swallows, such as the Striated Swallow (Hirundo striolata) in the TYPES OF PENSILE NESTS. i. Philippine Red Sunbird; 2. Cape Penduline Tit or " Kapok- vogel "; 3. Indian Tailor- bird; 4. Blood-breasted Flower-pecker. [.Copyright, Hutchinson & Co. 180 NEST OF CENTRAL- AMERICAN SWIFT. This species (Panyptila sancti-hieronymi) builds a nest not unlike that of a Weaver-bird in shape. THE BIRDS AS POTTERS 181 former country, and Fairy Martin (Lageno-plastes ariel) in the latter, make even better nests than the House-Martin, resembling mud jars attached by the base to their support, which is commonly a wall, these Swallows, like most of their group, readily availing themselves of man-made facilities. Three Australian birds, not apparently very nearly related and not at all like Swallows, the Grey Struthidea (Struthidea cinerea), White- winged Chough (Cor cor ax melanorbamphus), and Magpie- Lark or Pied Grallina (Grallina picata) distinguish themselves by making nests in the form of mud bowls, so extraordinarily true that if they were not attached to a support they could not be dis- tinguished from crude human-made pottery. Another wonderful mud- worker is the South American Oven-bird, whose domed nest, seated on a bough or post, has a side porch which renders invasion of the nesting chamber almost impossible. This is a very heavy nest, takes months to make, and lasts for years. The bird is a homely one, and a popular favourite, being called, like our Redbreast, by a Christian name, in this case " Alonzo Garcia " or " Alonzito," and credited with not working on Sundays, no doubt because it takes a spell off now and then. A close ally of this bird, curiously enough, is the burrowing " Minera," which reminds one of the great difference of the nesting-habits of the House- and Sand-Martins. A combination of hole-building and mud-masonry is found in the nest of the Nuthatch, which has the 1 84 BIRD BEHAVIOUR last sort of nest one would expect such a bird to make ; and among the Parrots, which have been accused of being too clumsy to build nests at all, the Quaker Parrakeet (Myopsittacus monachus) of South America not only builds a stick nest with roof and porch, but goes in for tenement nesting, several birds building in one clump, though the actual rooms are separate. A few non-Passerines build mud nests, such as that extraordinary fruit- eating ally of the Nightjars, the South American Guacharo (Steatornis cari- pensis), no doubt a primitive form, as its beak is less specialized or degenerate than a Nightjar's, and not so abnormally enlarged as a Frog-mouth's. Among the waders, Flamingoes and the Sun- Bittern also build mud nests, but in all these cases the nest is a mere cheese-shaped hassock, as it were, displaying no special merit in architecture. It is interesting, however, to find that other birds than Passerines have so much constructive ability, clumsy or not ; and in this connection it should be noted that the Broadbills, Passerines of a very primitive and clumsy type, build pendent nests nearly as good as those of Weavers. Nor can we say that birds of special intelligence will neces- sarily build an elaborate nest, for the clever Crow tribe, with the exception of the common Magpie with its domed nest of thorny branches, build nests of the most ordinary type for passerine birds. Parasitic nesting is extremely common among birds, especially among hole-building species which 5 "3 FLAMINGO ON ITS NEST. Showing the sitting position, which is rarely assumed by the Flamingo except when incubating. SWIFTLETS AND THEIR NESTS. These Swiftlets are the smallest of the family, not being so large as the Sand-martin among the Swallows. IN SEARCH OF LODGINGS 185 do not bore for themselves or are anxious to avoid the trouble ; thus many birds avail themselves gladly of the nest-borings of Woodpeckers, from Tits to tree-breeding Ducks such as the Golden- eye and Carolina. Such Ducks, with Owls and Kestrels, are among the most inveterate of nesting parasites, few of these birds having much notion of making a nest for itself. They are often not particular about a hole, but will adopt an open nest ; thus, the Long-eared Owl gladly utilizes the shallow nest of a Wood- Pigeon. The hole-builders indeed often act on the prin- ciple of " any port in a storm " ; they will parasitize mammals as well as other birds, since the Stock- Dove, Tawny Owl, Sheldrake, and Puffin all utilize rabbit-holes in this country, and in America the Burrowing-Owl inhabits those of the prairie- marmot — the so-called prairie-dog (Cynomys lu- dovicianus) — in the northern part of the western continent, and of a much bigger rodent, the vizcacha (Lagostomus trichodactylus) in the southern hemi- sphere. In England the Sparrow-Hawk, generally, unlike the Kestrel, an independent nest-builder, has been found nesting on top of the drey of a Squirrel which was occupied at the time by the little rodent, which reminds one of a converse case recorded in Argentina, when an opossum, a great foe of birds, had established itself in one of the rooms of a com- posite nest of the Quaker Parrakeet above alluded to, the stout walls and overhanging eaves preventing 1 86 BIRD BEHAVIOUR him from clearing the colony, which included besides the rightful owners a tree-building Teal (of some species) as an additional but harmless parasite. The Sparrow, as we all know, too often evicts the House-Martin and takes possession, but it is not so familiar a fact that the Swift does the same thing by him ; this seems not to have been recorded since the days of Gilbert White, but it used to happen regularly under the eaves of Mr. B. Clarke's photographic studio at Maidstone when I was a boy at school there with his sons. In Argentina the Oven-bird's cosy nest is coveted by a Saffron- Finch (Sy calls pelzelni) and a Tree- Martin (Progne tapera), this powerful Swallow even fighting the owners for its possession, and sometimes successfully ; and, strangest of all, the Wood- Sandpiper (Totanus glareola) and some allied species of Sandpipers breed in old nests of other birds and of squirrels. The remarkable phenomenon of parasitism in the young of birds has been known in the case of the common European Cuckoo from classical times, and is quite proverbial, though even in the habits and procedure of this well-known bird there are many points yet to be made out ; but it occurs not only in many other Cuckoos — though only in those of the Old World, and not universally there — but has originated independently in the Cow-birds, Pas- serines belonging to the family of Troupials (Icte- which is intermediate between the Finches PARASITIC FAMILIES 187 and the Starlings in form and habits, and in the curious little birds known as Honey-guides (Indi- catoridte), closely allied in their anatomy to the Barbets, and best known from Africa, though a few occur in South-east Asia and its islands. Among the Cuckoos, only those are parasitic which are essentially perchers, having short legs and powerful wings, and seldom coming to the ground ; among these the habit seems to be uni- versal in the Old World, from the tiny and beautiful Violet Cuckoo (Chrysococcyx amethystinus) of Malay- sia, to the huge Toucan-like Channel-bill (Scythrops novce-hollan&ice) of Australasia, though the American Cuckoos of this type do not display it. The short- winged, strong-legged, Magpie-like Bush-Cuckoos, such as the " Crow-Pheasant " of India and the Guira of South America, are never parasitic, though the latter and its black allies the Tick-birds (Croto- phaga) indulge in the custom of communal building and nestling- rearing. The general habits of the European Cuckoo are well known ; the egg, which is very small for the size of the bird, is usually laid on the ground and then taken in the bird's bill and placed in the foster-nest, though when this is conveniently situated, the Cuckoo may sit on it to lay her egg. The male may accompany the female to guard her or distract the hostility of the owners, but this is apparently exceptional, the general impression among naturalists being that Cuckoos live in a state of promiscuity and do not regard conjugal 1 88 BIRD BEHAVIOUR ties.' The natives of Yarkand go further, and say there are no male Cuckoos at all, the hens being the paramours of a Shrike (Lanius isabellinus), the Barred Warbler (Sylvia nisoria), or even of frogs ! a belief which at any rate argues that two Cuckoos ar^not often seen together. (The egg, when incubated by the fosterer, develops after a proportionately short incubation period into a blind naked young bird which has a hollow in the back, facilitating the work in which it almost immediately engages of turning eggs or its foster- nest-mates out of the nest by getting under them and backing upwards to the edge of the nest till they are rolled out. When, as rarely happens, two Cuckoos' eggs have been deposited in the same nest, the " survival of the fittest " is naturally not determined till after a severe contest. But this fury for eviction only lasts about a week, after which time the young Cuckoo will tolerate a bed- fellow. (The foster-parents do not concern themselves about this fratricidal behaviour in their nursery, but assiduously feed their changeling and leave the rightful heirs to die ; nor is there anything wonder- ful in this behaviour, as it is the common custom of birds to neglect a fallen nestling and to give most food to that which is most strongand ravenous) I once had an opportunity of watching a nestful of Starlings which were being reared on a roof just under my window at Oxford ; and as far as I could see there was not the least attempt at fair AN IRRESISTIBLE BEGGAR 189 feeding on the part of the parents. A very ravenous young bird appeared always to be on top of the rest, and to get nearly all contributions till he subsided and another got the opportunity of taking his place. The Calcutta pair of Dabchicks, how- ever, fed their young fairly, and I have seen a young bird pecked instead of fed when it clamoured too soon for a second helping. The fact that the young Cuckoo is assiduously fed long after it can fly is probably due to the insistent quality of its appeal ; birds which have not reared one will feed it, I and on one occasion when a young Cuckoo was confined in a Parrot- cage in the inside compartment of the Western Aviary at the Zoo, a Black Tanager (Tacbyphonus melanoleucus), a South American species which could have had no knowledge of such a bird, never- theless squeezed through the bars to feed it. The range of foster-parents in the case of the common Cuckoo is wider than in any other, and may partly account for the wide range of the species itself, Cuckoos generally being quite as tropical as Parrots or Humming-birds, though in view of the extension into North America of the non-parasitic Cuckoos, this is evidently not the only explanation. The favourite fosterer is the Titlark or Meadow- Pipit, and presumably it is due to its dependence on this bird that the Cuckoo ranges out on to the moorlands, a habitat for which it, like most of the family, is ytterly unsuited, being an awkward mover on the ground, though on trees it hops more actively 190 BIRD BEHAVIOUR than its short legs would lead one to expect.. The Hedge-Sparrow is, however, a well-known victim of the Cuckoo, and is the most often spoken of, since as it is such a familiar bird itself, the alien nestling is most often seen in its easily-found nest in most places^ Other well-known Cuckoo-rearers are the Robin, Pied Wagtail, and Reed- Warbler, and many other small birds are patronized, but chiefly the insec- tivorous kinds, though now and then a Finch's nest may be selected. Larger birds are very seldom Cuckoo-fosterers, probably because they can drive off the intruder — otherwise we should expect to find the Blackbird, Thrush, and Starling the commonest fosterers, their nests being easy to find and their habits in every way suitable. The Red-backed Shrike, however, in spite of its ferocity, is a not uncommon foster-parent on the Continent, though rarely so in this country. In this case, the egg of the Cuckoo seems gener- ally to bear a close resemblance to that of the Shrike, but such resemblance to the egg of the fosterer is not by any means universal. The Cuckoo's egg is indeed very variable, though not nearly so much so as that of the Guillemot ; and its small size relatively to that of its producer is paralleled by the similar smallness of the egg of the Raven above alluded to. The mottled-drab coloration which is the most usual is certainly not very unlike that of Wagtails' and Pipits' eggs, though the resemblance is far closer to that of the MIMICRY IN EGG-COLOUR 191 Skylark than to any other bird's, but no Robin or Hedge-Sparrow, even if colour-blind, could fail to notice the difference of the alien egg. (It is said that eggs resembling those of the foster- parents, such as the blue form found in the Red,- start's nest, tend to be confined to fosterers which are rarely patronized, leading to the inference that such are not easily duped, so that a perfect imitation has been evolved ; but such an explanation involves the assumptions that certain strains of Cuckoos always lay in particular birds' nests, and always produce eggs of the same colour, of which there is no evidence at present. Certainly, as in the case of Tree-Sparrows and Eagles, the same individual birds do not always lay similar eggs ; a very remarkable instance of this has occurred in the case of the Nightingale, in whose nest have been found an egg of the normal olive-brown, a blue one, and two of intermediate shades. In some Oriental Cuckoos there is a very perfect assimilation between the egg of the parasite and the fosterer ; the " Brain-fever-bird " (Hierococcyx varius) and the Pied Crested Cuckoo (Coccystes jacobinus) lay in the nests of Babbling-Thrushes (Crateropus and Argya) and their eggs are plain blue like those of the Babblers ; and the Koel (Eudynamis honor ata), the commonest and best- known Cuckoo in India, is parasitic on House and Jungle Crows (Corvus splendens and C. culminatus), and lays an egg very like a small Crow's egg. It is doubtful, however, whether the imitation is here 192 BIRD BEHAVIOUR of any service, for just as Graham records in the " Birds of lona and Mull " that he got Hooded Crows to hatch Bantam eggs smeared with indigo, so Mr. D. Dewar has got the House-Crow to hatch an ordinary Hen's egg. In the Koel, by the way, the parasitism is not so perfect as in the common Cuckoo ; the young bird does not always eject the young Crows, and the old birds feed it after it has been reared in the Crows' nest. I have myself, however, seen the Crows feeding a young Koel, which is curious, as they hate and persecute the old bird. The Koel avails itself of this to get its egg deposited, the male bird drawing off the Crows in pursuit of him, while the female deposits her egg, presumably laying it in the nest, this being convenient, while the bird, like so many tropical fruit-eaters, seems never to come to the ground, and so is not likely to descend in order to lay there. Another case of incomplete parasitism among Cuckoos is recorded of an African species, the splendidly glittering little Emerald Cuckoo (Chryso- coccyx smaragdineus), which Keulemans, the late well-known bird artist, told Buller, as related by the latter in his " Birds of New Zealand," is in the habit of hatching its solitary egg, and then leaving the young bird to the mercy of the bird public ; passing birds, he said, attracted by the cry of the Cuckoo nestling, dropped contributions into its mouth, an episode he had himself often witnessed on Prince's Island. \ TOLERATION IN A CUCKOO 193 An allied but less brilliant species, one of the Bronze Cuckoos (C. cupreus), also African, is parasitic on such different small birds as Sun-birds and Finches ; a similar species, the Shining Cuckoo (C. lucidus) of New Zealand, generally lays its egg in the nest of the small Grey Warbler (Gerygone flaviventris), which makes a pensile nest with side entrance. This same little bird is parasitized by the other New Zealand Cuckoo (Urodynamis taitensis), which is larger than our Cuckoo, while the Shining Cuckoo is much smaller. This large New Zealand Cuckoo, however, has also been found in the nest of the Wood-Robin (Miro albifrons), and it was particularly noted that although it throve well, and ultimately sat on top of the young Robins, these also lived, and when the young Cuckoo and a young Robin were removed and caged, the old Robins fed both of them. When a nest of the Grey Warbler containing a young Shining Cuckoo was watched in the same country, however, it was noticed that the young Warblers, although not immediately at all events ejected by their bedfellow, nevertheless died off one by one, and some were found outside. The large New Zealand Cuckoo is extending its parasitism to the introduced British birds, and also preys on their young, as it did on those of the native birds, and their eggs, when first studied ; these predatory habits make it less surprising that our Cuckoo should be credited with eating some of the eggs of the foster-parents in whose nests 13 194 BIRD BEHAVIOUR it deposits its own. Many must be familiar with the old rhyme about the Cuckoo, about which it is said that She sucks little birds' eggs To make her voice clear. A taste for the eggs of other birds is not at all uncommon among the insectivorous species — at any rate it frequently develops in captivity, and as to the antiquity of the tradition, it must be remembered that formerly Cuckoos were evidently far more familiar to the British public than they are now, judging by the allusions to them in our literature. Shakespeare in the " Midsummer Night's Dream " makes Bully Bottom the Weaver allude to the " plain-song Cuckoo grey." I fancy most people nowadays, whether weavers or writers, would not be able to say off-hand of what colour a Cuckoo is. The colour of the young Cuckoo, however, with its black barring on a ground of brown above and whitish below, is evidently pretty familiar, this changeling being so often found in the nest of its fosterer, and exposing itself when fledged quite freely until it leaves us in the autumn, in a manner very unlike the stealthy habits of its parents in the spring ; so that it is not astonishing that " Cuckoo " is the poultry- fancier's term for a barred grey fowl such as the Plymouth-rock, though any brown shade in this breed would now be a disqualification. It may be mentioned that there are wild birds O rfs o §•§ g 3 M o SI M w ^ tf) ... f i & o I CUCKOOS AND THEIR MODELS 195 coloured just like a barred grey fowl of ordinary type and not of very high standard of marking, and that these are the grey species of the flightless Kiwis or Apteryxes. The coincidence of such a peculiar marking occurring in two such very different birds as the flightless nocturnal New Zealand Kiwi and varieties of our domestic fowls, should prepare us for coincidences in coloration, and help to make us sceptical about the survival value of the curious resemblances to Hawks seen in many of the Cuckoos. Some of these are cer- tainly most extraordinary ; the Brain-fever-bird or Hawk-Cuckoo of India above mentioned is extra- ordinarily like the common Hawk of the Indian plains known as the Shikra (Astur badius) and the resemblance is not confined to the adult birds, but extends to the young in first plumage, which are differently clad from them in both mimic and model. Another member of the genus Hiero- coccyx to which this bird belongs, H. sfarverioides, resembles another Sparrow-Hawk, the Besra (Acci- fiter virgatus). (The Hawk-like appearance of our own Cuckoo has often been commented on, but it is very sketchy compared with that of the above species, and the young and adult stages do not resemble the same kinds of Hawks, and the shape is less Sparrow-Hawk- like. Moreover, the young of the non-parasitic Crow-Pheasant is just as much like a Hawk, when of the barred type (some resemble the black, chestnut- winged adult, but are usually duller), as our young 196 BIRD BEHAVIOUR Cuckoo, and I have in Calcutta seen tame Gtimea- Fowls and wild Crows distinctly impressed and apparently alarmed by one of these young birds which came into my possession, just as in the Zoo the keepers find that their small birds from many countries kept in the West Aviary are frightened if a common Cuckoo is introduced to them. A fairly good general resemblance is therefore sufficient to impress birds, but the point of it seems to be wanting. If the Hawk-like appear- ance of certain Cuckoos scares the fated fosterers from their nest, what end is served by the Hawk- like appearance of the fostered young ? Besides, many parasitic Cuckoos are not like Hawks, or resemble Hawks found in a different country. Thus, the large New Zealand Cuckoo, as noted by Buller, is extremely like an American Hawk (Acci- fiter cooperi), but does not so closely resemble any native Hawk ; and the same Babblers which foster the Brain-fever-bird also rear, as has been said, the pied Crested Cuckoo which, with its crested head and plumage black above and white below, is like no Indian Hawk. Neither is any Hawk anywhere like the splendid Emerald, Bronze, and Violet Cuckoos, for adult Hawks and other birds of prey, like the downy young birds we have been considering, follow mammalian rules of colour, and eschew brilliant tints except on bare parts. Some even of the non-parasitic Cuckoos may have a rather Hawk-like coloration, like the large fruit- eating Cuckoo, Carpococcyx fadiatus, of the WIDE-SPREAD CUCKOO-PATTERN 197 Malayan islands, which is indeed rather Pheasant- like in shape, but Hawk-like in pattern. The curious resemblance between most parasitic Cuckoos and the Hawks in having long thigh-plumes must be pure coincidence, as the short legs of the Cuckoos are not suited for exhibiting this point, and besides, the Hawks they most resemble are long on the leg ; Hawks also sit erect, not horizontally like Cuckoos. It must not be forgotten that the Sparrow-Hawk pattern, or Cuckoo pattern, whichever one chooses to call it, of a plain or nearly plain upper surface and a barred lower side, is one of the most strikingly recurrent patterns in the bird class, like the Magpie pattern to which Mr. D. Dewar and myself have drawn attention in our critical work on "The Making of Species." It is found, for instance, among the Passerines, in many of the thence-named Cuckoo- Shrikes (Campepbaga) ; in the male Barred Warbler of Europe (Sylvia nisoria) ; in both sexes of a little Australian Finch, the Cherry-crowned (Aictemosyne modesta) ; in an Australian Duck, the Pink-eyed (Malacorhynchus membranaceus) ; in the female of the well-known Upland Goose (Chloephaga magellanica) — which also happens to have the gamboge-yellow feet so common in birds of prey, and so rare elsewhere, instead of the orange not uncommon in waterfowl — and in both sexes of an allied compatriot, the Ruddy-headed Goose (C. rubi- diceps) ; in an Indian Owl (Glaucidium cuculoides), and in some little Doves jDf the genus Geopelia. 198 BIRD BEHAVIOUR The most fervent advocate of mimicry will hardly maintain that small Doves and Passerines and big waterfowl mimic Hawks ; so why not put the whole thing down to coincidence ? The resem- blance of our Cuckoo to a Hawk does not save it from the real bird, for Mr. J. H. Owen in " British Birds " for 1914 expressly mentions Cuckoos among the victims of the Sparrow-Hawk. The Honey-guides, which are a family of very few species, appear to be all parasitic, but practically nothing is known about the ways of the Asiatic species, not even the extraordinary and well-known instinct of guiding men and the Ratel or Honey- badger (Mellivora) to bee- nests being recorded of them. The egg of the African Honey-guides, which is white, has been found in the nest of the White-throated Swallow (Hirundo albigularis) by Mr. Ivy in the case of Sparrmann's Honey-guide (Indicator sparrmanni) as recorded in Stark and Sclater's " Birds of South Africa " ; the same gentleman recorded the egg of the Yellow- throated species (/. major) in the nest of a Drongo-shrike ; while Mrs. Barber found the same species parasitizing the Black-collared Barbet (Lybius torquatus). She also found the Lesser Honey-guide (/. minor) parasitizing the Tinker, another Barbet of very small size (Barbatula pusilla). Mr. Ivy found a pair of the above-mentioned Black-collared Barbet actually trying to fight off this Honey-guide from their nesting-hole, the intruder persistently returning ; evidently her case NESTLING HONEY-GUIDES 199 was a desperate one, for when she was shot the egg was found to be actually half-laid. No doubt it is an emergency like this in the case of the common Cuckoo which accounts for the egg being found in such unlikely and unsuitable nests as those of the Wood-Pigeon and, of all others, the Dabchick ! The strong tendency of the Honey-guides to sponge on their own relatives, the Barbets, is noticeable ; the present species has been said to lay in the nests not only of those mentioned, but also in those of the Pied Barbet (Tricbol&ma leuco- melari) and of a Woodpecker (Mesopicus griseo- cefhalus). It is of interest here to note that the plumage of the Honey-guides is plain and ordinary in character in all the species, resembling that of the less con- spicuous Finches, and not in the least like the striking and often variegated tints of the fosterers they select. They appear, however, always to use the nests of birds which lay white eggs, though their own are distinguishable from these readily enough. The young Honey-guide has the ends of both jaws sharply hooked, which is not the case with the adult ; it is supposed that this structure is comparable to the hollowed back of the young Cuckoo, an adaptation to facilitate the ejection of the foster-fellow-nestlings, in this case to ensure a strong grip with the beak. The young Honey- guide certainly seems when found to have the nest to itself, like some young Cuckoos and Cow-birds, but it must be remembered with regard to the 200 BIRD BEHAVIOUR structure of the beak that this is in other cases sometimes more hooked in the young than in the adult, as in young Gannets and Herons ; and incidentally it would be well to examine all young Cuckoos available, to see if the non-parasitic species were hollow-backed at first as well as the others. The old Honey-guides appear to associate with the young when reared, and perhaps instruct them unconsciously or otherwise ; Mr. Ivy found two old and three young birds of the Scaly-throated species (/. variegatus) together, of which only one old bird would guide him. The parasitism of the Cow-birds is in a way more instructive than that of the Cuckoos and Honey-guides, because here the gradations of the instinct can be very plainly traced. These birds, which are among the most Finch-like of the Trou- pials, having short thick bills, and being able to live on dry seed which they crack in typical Finch fashion, are all closely related and easily recognizable. One of them, the original Cow-bird (Molobrus -pecans) of North America, is as truly and com- pletely parasitical as our Cuckoo, laying one egg in the nests of various small insectivorous birds, which rear that young bird alone, so that the others evidently perish, even if not ejected ; a fate which is likely to befall weak young birds in any case, as exemplified by my Canary experience. This is a glossy-black species with a sooty-brown head in the male, brown in the female, and used to consort with bisons when they were common, as it now INCOMPETENT COW-BIRDS 201 does with cattle, whence its scientific and popular names. Like the rest of the genus, it is chiefly a ground-bird and a walker, so that in general habits it presents no resemblance whatever to the parasitic Cuckoos. In South America are found several species, and it is to the able historian of the birds of Argentina Mr. W. H. Hudson, that we owe the elucidation of their curious half- developed instincts. The best- known species, the glossy Cow-bird (Molobrus bonar- iensis), is very common and often imported here as a cage-bird ; the male is a resplendent purple and the hen sooty. It is truly parasitic in that its young are always reared by other birds, but its parasitism is slovenly and incomplete. Several females will lay in the same foster-nest, and fairly swamp it with their undesired contributions, whereas it is rare in the case of the Cuckoo to find, two parasite eggs in one nest. To this suicidal instinct they add another equally silly, of destroying any sort of eggs they may come across, those of their own species included ; and moreover they recklessly drop eggs about on the ground, a habit shared by another South American bird, the Rhea. And just as the hen Rheas are, so to speak, parasitic on their own males, and pool their eggs in his nest, so these hen Cow- birds occasionally make a futile nest of their own, in some silly place such as on the leaves of a large thistle, and leave even this poor attempt unfinished. Then there is a species, brown with chestnut 202 BIRD BEHAVIOUR wings in both sexes (M. badius), which is also common, and is not parasitic, except to the mild extent of stealing another bird's nest when it can ; this species is itself parasitized by another (M. rufo-axillaris) in which both sexes are black. I particularize the colours of these species because it is interesting to note that the one with the simplest and probably most primitive coloration retains its primitive habits, and the most advanced in coloration is actually parasitic on this; a fact which, however curious it may appear, is not so paradoxical after all, for allied species recognize each other's affinity, and as long as a member of a parasitical group retained the ordinary nesting- habits of birds, it is, after all, at least as reasonable that a parasite should quarter itself on this as on an alien. There is, however, no case known of a parasitic Cuckoo quartering its offspring on an independent one, although species of both types are habitually found in the same countries in the Old World. The one Cow-bird which penetrates into the northern half of the American continent is, like our exceptionally widely- ranging Cuckoo, a bird of extended parasitism, favouring various species of small Finches and insectivorous birds ; with the Cat-bird (Galeoscoptes carolinensis), a larger species than most of its fosterers, it is seldom successful, and the Golden Warbler (Dendrceca estiva) some- times disposes of the Cow-bird's egg, not by turning it out of the nest, but by building a fresh floor over THE DRONGO AND ITS MIMIC 203 it, and in the case of a repeated invasion by the parasite, may have recourse to this expedient more than once. Another case of parasitism, and in this case also on allies, is furnished by the Troupials, the large black Cassidix oryzivora, a bird about the size of a Jackdaw, being a parasite on some of the other Troupials known as Hang-nests. Cow-birds do not mimic their dupes in appear- ance any more than Honey-guides ; and indeed this resemblance to an alien fosterer only appears in one or two Cuckoos, and is doubtful there. One Eastern Cuckoo, indeed (Surniculus dicruroides), has been called the Drongo-Cuckoo, and attained a great celebrity in books and museums, owing to its resemblance to the common, conspicuous, and highly pugnacious black Drongo-Shrikes (Dicrurus), which it is believed to parasitize, though the chief evidence of this is that a pair of Drongos has been seen to kill one of these Cuckoos, which does not look as if the resemblance did much good to its possessor. The size and general shape of Drongo and Drongo-Cuckoo are certainly very similar, but after all there are plenty of birds of which this could be said ; the black colour goes for nothing, as there are several other black Cuckoos, parasitic and otherwise, such as Cuculus clamosus allied to our Cuckoo, Coccystes serratus among the crested para- sitic Cuckoos, and the whole genus Crotopbaga or Anis among the non-parasites. Moreover, the tail 204 BIRD BEHAVIOUR of the Drongo-mimic is only square or slightly forked, whereas the tail of the Drongo itself has an extra strong and deep fork with an outward turn like that of our Blackcock, so that these spirited Shrikes would have to be uncommonly bad observers not to detect the impostor at a glance. However that may be, the other case does not seem very much better ; one of the Koels of the genus Eudynamis allied to the Indian Koel above mentioned lays in Borneo in the nest of a Talking Mynah (Eulabes), and the young of both sexes are said to be black, as males of this genus of Cuckoos always are, the hens being speckled like hen Pheasants. This is supposed to deceive the parent Mynahs, which are black birds, while the Cuckoos are growing up, the hen getting her colour later on. Something similar has been said about the Indian Koel, which, as we have seen, parasitizes Crows, birds which are certainly more clever than any Mynah ; but as a matter of fact, in Bengal at any rate, young Koels do not resemble the male in all cases. Young cocks are black, indeed, but have some buffy markings, and young hens are almost as variegated as the old birds, equally so in fact save for a solid black cap. It would surely be far more to the point for the hen to be like a Crow, so as to approach the nest in greater safety, if evolution here were proceeding on orthodox lines. And as to the resemblance of the young to that of fosterers, the^Hawk-like young of other Cuckoos manage to A FUTILE RESEMBLANCE 205 get brought up all right, in spite of the absence of resemblance to the fosterers' -true young, and of the presence of a resemblance to the most hated and feared of birds. Resemblances to some other bird than a near ally seem to be a marked peculiarity of the Cuckoo family ; not only do some of the parasitic Cuckoos look like Hawks, and at least one, as we have seen, rather like a Drongo, but the largest of this type, the Channel-bill, is so like a Hornbill that it was early described as one — yet there are no Hornbills in Australia, where this bird occurs, and the nearest Hornbill, found in New Guinea, is not that which the Channel-bill resembles, the likeness being to some of the small grey Indian species. A parasitic Cuckoo may even resemble a non- parasitic one, as in the case of the crested Coccystes coromandus, which is coloured just like a non- parasitic " Crow-Pheasant " (Centropus monacbus), though the former is Indian and the latter African, so that an excellent case of what is called in insects " Mullerian mimicry " falls to the ground. It would be just as well for parasitic Cuckoos if they did get mistaken for stronger birds, as they are often bullied, even by species which they do not infest, but from what has been said, there does not seem to be any really definite development of this mimicry in appearance. CHAPTER VII Migration — An anciently observed phenomenon still imperfectly understood — Reasons for it — Methods as far as is known — Difference between migratory species and the homing Pigeon — Widespread tendency to migration, contrasted with con- tradictory tendency to form localized non-migratory races, ending in some cases in Sightlessness, as in some birds of remote islands MIGRATION has always been a conspicuous pheno- menon in the lives of certain birds, and often, as in the case of Wildfowl and Quails, intimately bound up with the question of human food-supplies, so> that it is a very familiar fact ; but in spite of this; familiarity, it is still far from being properly under- stood. The old idea of the hibernation of some birds is indeed definitely done away with, and nowadays no one seems to believe that small birds* ever ride on big ones ; yet Bee-eaters in Africa1, have been found riding on Storks or Bustards when, these were searching for prey on the ground, and taking their share of the insects driven up ; so- where is the difficulty of a weary Wren or Warbler taking refuge on a Swan or Gannet, which certainly could not dislodge it in mid-air ? I mention this because there is no physical impossibility involved, 206 A MIGRANT IN MINIATURE 207 as in the case of Swallows, which were supposed to winter under water, and the case is worth con- sidering. As to the reasons for migration, we have here some chance, when we know more completely the life- history of the birds, of finding them out ; and at the outset, migration is in its essence simply the periodical removal of an animal from conditions which have ceased to be attractive to a locality where it is, or expects to be, more comfortable. It is not confined to birds, for some fish are well- known migrants — one has only to cite the salmon — • and mammals also undertake migrations, as the American bison used to do, and as sea-lions do still. A very interesting case is that of a small and very local British land-snail (Helicodonta obvoluta), now only found here on a limited area on the South Downs, which executes a miniature migra- tion from its hibernating haunt on the ground to the branches of the beech-trees in spring, spending the summer aloft, and in autumn coming down the trunk to bury itself again. There is no essential difference between the short journey of this humble mollusc and the enormous transit of the world's record migrant, the Arctic Tern, which breeds up to the northern limit of land, and in our winter reaches, on its southern migrations, even to the cheerless shores of the Antarctic continent, there to enjoy whatever sort of summer that unpromising region can offer it. Migrations in elevation, made no doubt very 208 BIRD BEHAVIOUR often with little or no use of the wings in the case of ground birds, are quite the normal thing in hill- living birds, as in so many species inhabiting the Himalayas ; the splendid Monaul or Impeyan Pheasant (Lophophorus refulgens), for instance, ranges up to the very edge of the forest belt in summer, and comes down in winter into the lower deciduous woods, where there is more chance of food. Wild Turkeys in America used to wander long distances in search of mast, acorns, etc., though their powers of flight are so limited that in crossing a river a mile broad, some of the weaker birds were certain to fall in and have to finish the transit as best they could by swimming, and it took days of gobbling exhortation from the old cocks to get the emigrants' spirit up to the pitch of starting at all. Want of food is obviously the chief reason why birds of high elevations or high latitudes have to leave their haunts ; cold is by itself very little operative, for it is not surprising that birds prove comparatively indifferent to it, considering their naturally high temperature and particularly warm clothing. Not only do numerous tropical Finches and Parrakeets winter safely in outdoor aviaries, but of our poultry the majority come from hot climates, the Fowl and Peacock, for instance, being typical inhabitants of the plains and foothills of India. With the end of autumn, however, comes not only scarcity of food, but absence of cover, owing to the deciduous character of so much temperate vegetation, so that both for fear of BRAVING OUT THE WINTER 209 getting nothing to eat and of becoming food for others, the birds of temperate climates have to seek a warmer country for the winter. Species whose habits render them independent of leaves and herbage and fresh water tend to be resident ; thus, the Eider-Duck and Razor-bill will winter on the edge of the Arctic ice, and Woodpeckers and Tits, whose food is sought on boughs and trunks rather than on leaves, can pass the winter in leafless forests that Warblers and Cuckoos must leave, while the Grouse feed on conifer- needles and buds or burrow in the snow for their food. Berry- eating birds like Thrushes can live till the supply is gone, and a confirmed berry-eater, the Waxwing, roves about the north all winter, its long wings, almost Swallow- like in form, giving it ranging powers that enable it to dispense with a distant southern journey in many years, though it may even reach Northern India. Some birds of prey, too, remain even in the high north as long as there is anything to be picked up, such as the Snowy Owl, though even this has strayed south to the Punjab. The universality of the migration depends of course on local conditions ; thus in the compara- tively mild climate of our islands many species are to be seen throughout the year which are migratory in corresponding latitudes on the Continent, the Robin and Song-Thrush for instance. Swallows often remain after their time of leaving, and, as Gilbert White observed, they stay particularly late 2to BIRD BEHAVIOUR about Oxford, and I have been the unwilling witness of the gradual failure and death, evidently from hunger, of some of these poor would-be colonists in permanency. Birds of passage in the Southern Hemisphere naturally migrate north in winter, from the same motives as induce the northern birds to go south, but there is a curious exception in the case of the Emperor Penguin, which chooses mid-winter in its haunts on the Antarctic ice-floes as the time for laying and rearing its chick. Some other birds, too, seem to come into breeding condition at most unlikely-seeming times, for the fragile little Sun- birds in some cases breed in high mountainous parts of South Africa in winter when the snow lies quite deep under their hanging nests. Besides the direct north and south migrations also, there are cases where the migration is rather from east to west, as in the cases^of the White-eyed and Red-crested Pochards (Nyroca africana and Netta rufina), whose chief winter haunt is in India, though their breeding-range extends not only into Central Asia, but far west into Europe. The Rosy Pastor presents the unusual case of a bird which has a fixed wintering station in the east, and a pronounced westerly migration, but no definite breeding- haunt ; for these birds in winter are essentially Indian, but breed in Western Asia and Eastern Europe, but in different places in different years, the flocks, which keep together even in the breeding-season and nest in company, LIFE ON THE EASTERN ISLES 211 settling down to breed irregularly in different spots, being guided apparently by the abundance of the locusts, which form a favourite food. In the East Indies there are curious cases of inter-insular migration in at least two Pigeons, the ground- feeding Nicobar or Hackled Pigeon (Calcenas nicobarica) and the arboreal Pied Fruit-Pigeon (Myristicivora luctuosa), which wander far and wide in the many islands of the Indian Ocean, but do not come to the mainland. The case of the Nicobar Pigeon is particularly remarkable, as only scattered pairs are found breeding elsewhere than on Batty Malve, a small and fortunately very inaccessible islet in the Nicobar group, which appears to be a perfect Pigeon-house during the season when the birds rear their single chick. This regular return to isolated spots in huge numbers is almost unique in a land-bird, but, as is well known, quite a common thing among sea-fowl, especially Petrels and Penguins ; it was only com- paratively recently found out where one of the com- monest and most widespread among southern ocean Petrels, the well-known spotted " Cape Pigeon " (Daption capensis), nested, the locality being Ker- guelen. Wilson's Petrel (Oceanites oceanicus) also nests in Kerguelen, and spends the southern winter in the north, affording a rare instance of a southern- breeding bird crossing the equator, though it is quite the usual thing for migrants from north to south to go far south of this line. It has, indeed, been found that of migratory 212 BIRD BEHAVIOUR groups, the species which go farthest north to breed often winter farthest south, as exemplified among the Swifts and the Turtle-Doves ; among Ducks, too, the arctic-breeding Pin-tail and Wigeon go farther south than the temperate-zone-haunting Mallard. None of the Geese, however, which mostly breed in the high north, go very far south. Many naturalists seem puzzled as to why birds which have escaped starvation by going towards equatorial lands should retrace their course again to the countries they have left ; but this is because they have generally little practical acquaintance with any but the birds of the Northern Hemisphere, where only, in Europe and the United States, do ornithologists much abound. To any one who knows the tropics, the problem does not seem so wonderful. In the less well-watered parts of these countries, the heat of the hot season produces effects not so very different from the frosts of the temperate winter. Herbage and leaves are parched up, with the result that both insect and vegetable food become scarce, and the water supply is much restricted. Under these conditions reptiles, frogs, and even fish " aestivate," i.e. go in for a summer sleep, in the case of the aquatic creatures burying themselves in the mud, and native birds of aquatic habits have to leave these districts for better- watered ones. Obviously, then, the migrants are no better off, and have every reason to get out of a country in which the residents have all they can do to survive. HEAT AND ITS INFLUENCE 213 The heat itself may have something to do with making them leave, although on the whole birds from temperate countries or of mountaineering habits seem to bear heat as well as tropical resi- dents, judging from what I was able to observe at the Calcutta Zoo. This is a most remarkable fact, considering that the very peculiarities of birds I alluded to above as fitting them to withstand cold are just those which ought to make them susceptible to heat ; and it is a curious fact that the warmly-clad Duck tribe bear heat, even when migrants from the north, particularly well, while the thinly-clothed Pheasant family are more susceptible than any other birds ; the Monaul bears heat worse than any bird I know, panting in a temperature which provokes no such manifestation of oppression from any other bird. Among the Pigeons, too, I noted the Hima- layan Snow-Pigeon (Columba leuconota) showed no distress at the heat, although a high alpine bird, while some of the warm-climate-dwelling fruit- Pigeons were obviously affected, as were many Parrots. Crows also feel heat much, even tropical species like the Indian House-Crow, and the House- Sparrow, although it lives well through both heat and cold, pants constantly in the Indian summer, while the imported Goldfinch seems quite indifferent to heat. It is noteworthy, too, that while the Mallard in its wild state does not visit the tropics except on its southern migrations, its descendant, the domestic 214 BIRD BEHAVIOUR Duck, lives and breeds well there, the most spe- cialized of all its breeds, the Penguin Duck — apparently the ancestor of the so-calleld " Indian " Runner, so deservedly popular nowadays — being characteristic of the Malay Islands, so that, though even in this country young Ducklings are found to be very intolerant of heat, it must have been long and perfectly acclimatized in the East ; and many of the migratory Ducks I watched in confinement in Calcutta showed distinct signs of wishing to breed. On the whole, then, there is little evidence that the direct effect of heat itself has influenced the spring-time ebb of bird-life away from the tropics ; but its indirect effects, as hinted above, are another matter, and apropos of this it may be noted that in the Bombay famine of a few years ago the game-birds, which could not migrate, suffered and died just as if they were mammals, while better flyers escaped. Then there has to be taken into consideration the attitude of the bird-population of the tropics themselves. It is true that this is so numerous and varied that the huge tidal wave of migrants from the north into India makes no more obvious difference to the human observer of the bird population than does the influx of the football crowd into London on the occasion of the great Cup-tie contest to the apparent fulness of the metropolis, and no doubt things are much the same elsewhere. PRESSURE FROM RESIDENTS 215 But birds, like savage tribes and civilized nations, have boundaries to consider, and if they invade the territories of others, must sooner or later have to fight for a place or return whence they came. And in such a contest, which must be inevitable in the tropics when food and favourable locations get at all scarce, the advantage is all on the side of the residents, which are generally speaking birds of a superior type mentally and morally, even when less specialized physically, and perhaps, indeed, on that very account. They generally excel the birds of temperate regions in courage, social instincts, and intelligence, as is shown by their much greater power of combination, and by the far superior nests they often build. Finches of the typical temperate-zone groups have a poor time in an aviary if crowded along with Weavers, and the Tree-Ducks know how to make the migratory temperate-zone Ducks respect them, while the solitary Thrushes would have a poor chance against the Babblers, with their powers of combination which enable them often to resist even Hawks. There is a popular belief that birds in temperate climates sing better than in the tropics, but all the foundation for this idea is the fact that the Nightin- gales are birds of the temperate zone, and that some very common Thrushes are good singers ; with the exception of the Nightingales no singers of the north of the Old World can surpass, or even equal, the Shama (Cittocincla macrura) and Orange- headed Ground-Thrush (Geocichla citrina) of India, 2i6 BIRD BEHAVIOUR and in the New World the Mocking-birds (Mimus) are essentially southern, though the most celebrated (M. 'polyglottus) ranges into the northern states. The only point in which the birds of temperate climates really excel the tropical forms is in power of flight, which they have had perforce to cultivate, and even this superiority is not universal, for there are some of the most magnificent flyers among the Parrots and Pigeons, which are essentially tropical to say nothing of Frigate-birds and Tropic -birds. In addition to influences of this kind, which are operative on young and old birds alike, there is the instinctive attachment to the breeding-place, which is so strong in birds that they have often been recorded as returning to it for many years in suc- cession. This of course must act to a certain extent on the old birds, and even the young, if at all uncomfortable where they find themselves by the time they complete their first year, are most likely to come back to the place which gave them birth, and where they had their first insight into the enjoyments of flight and independence. The main point about migration, therefore, is the fixing of the methods, not the motive ; and it is here that we are confronted with problems difficult to solve. The case of the Homing Pigeon ought to give us some help, but such Pigeons are well known to be guided almost entirely by sight, and to need training by gradually increased stages ; whereas birds of passage frequently, perhaps most commonly, fly by night, and, in the autumn migra- THE HOMING INSTINCT 217 tion, the young often leave the land of their birth before the adults do. A converse case of juvenile independence is that of the common Cuckoo, the old birds leaving us in July, while the young remain till the general southward movement of migrants in September, long after all old Cuckoos have left us. That many birds have some method of correctly directing their course without the aid of sight is shown not only by the night-flights of many, but by the return of Penguins to their breeding-grounds-, unable as they necessarily are to take a wide survey, owing to their flightlessness ; by the return of Noddy and Sooty Terns to their breeding-places in the Southern United States after they had been taken away and released hundreds of miles north of their range, and by at least two instances of flying birds taking the correct direction on foot when deprived of the power of flight, that of a Canada Goose recorded by Audubon, and of an Upland Goose cited by Mr. Hudson — though in the latter case it must be admitted that the bird's full-winged mate was with it. These cases are, however, par- ticularly interesting in that in the former the bird's spring destination was north, and in the latter south. In England, also, a cock Reeves's Pheasant, released thirty miles away from home, was found next day perched on the aviary in which he had been bred. There is undoubtedly a great deal of more or less random movement, especially in young birds ; 2i 8 BIRD BEHAVIOUR such are particularly apt to appear in localities not normally frequently by their species, as I found in the case of the invasion of India in the late 'nineties by Baer's White- eyed Pochard (Nyroca baeri), most of the specimens I obtained being young birds. Birds also often range for a time north of their breeding-haunts in autumn, and may even fly out into the Atlantic from our coasts, only to perish or come back utterly exhausted. There is thus a good deal to be said in justification for the statement that young birds " wander almost at random," and that most of them are probably destroyed before the next spring ; and the dangers of a long migration may be the chief reason why birds are more numerous, but less fertile, in the tropics, where there is not this wholesale risk to be faced annually. Of course there is a. good deal of local movement even in the tropics, but not much is known about this as yet, and the journeys are inevitably far less risky. It has been pointed out that the reason why day-birds so often migrate by night is that by so doing they utilize hours which would in any case have to be spent without food, and thus economize time and strength ; but the ever-present danger of Gulls, Carrion-Hawks, and Crows, some or other of which are always to be found along every coast, all on the alert to note signs of weariness in birds they ordinarily would have no hope of capturing, must surely have something to do with this, and it is to be noted that in all groups the larger or RELINQUISHING TRAVEL 219 more powerfully-flying species are more inclined to day migration than the feebler folk; thus, Crows and Swallows among the Passerines, and Geese and Swans among the wildfowl, migrate by day, as do Hawks, Storks, and other powerful and predatory birds. The tendency to migration is nearly universal among birds of temperate zones ; even, as re- marked above, in species not usually thought of as migrants, because always visible at any time of year in our country at least ; the most thoroughly sedentary of flying birds being the Game-birds and Woodpeckers, which have special facilities for finding food, though even of these some migrate, such as the Quail and Greater Spotted Woodpecker. On the other hand, there is a strong tendency for migrants to settle down and form non-migratory local races ; thus, most of our resident British birds are recognizably different from the conti- nental individuals of the same species which visit us, and are nowadays distinguished as local races or sub-species ; and we may notice that the Blackbird shows a much greater tendency to become per- manently resident in the north than the Thrush, a weaker and less versatile but more strongly-flying bird. The Quail, one of the most anciently-known migrants, often used to pass the winter here when it was commoner, and conversely has been known to breed in numbers in India, usually only a winter- resort, while it has established resident races in Spain and in the Canaries, although attempts to 220 BIRD BEHAVIOUR introduce it artificially into the United States have failed. This tendency to settle down and have done with the fatigues and dangers of migration has resulted in the production of many localized and often insular races of birds, which have often become flightless, where flight was of little import- ance in their daily life ; thus we had the Great Auk in the north, formerly a most abundant bird, and in the Galapagos there has been discovered of late years a flightless Cormorant (Nannopterum harrisi), and on the elevated Lake Titicaca in the Peruvian Andes there lives a flightless Grebe (Podicipes mi crofter us), such a lake being the bio- logical equivalent of an oceanic island. Such flightless species are to be found most com- monly amongst the Rails, birds which migrate a good deal, but are nevertheless poor flyers as a whole, and always more ready to use their legs than their wings ; they also have a most peculiar pro- pensity for turning up in the most out-of-the-way places, being probably unable to make head against a wind, while their power of swimming enables them to rest on the water and gain fresh strength where others would drown. The widest migrant and most successful bird of all is the little unspecialized Turnstone, which is to be found on all shores in the world at one time or other of the year, and is strongly sus- pected of having established^ breeding colonies far and wide in the world, though its main haunts THE SUCCESS OF THE TERNS 221 in the nesting-season are undoubtedly in the north. When we come to consider groups, however, the palm for success in the struggle for existence, as evidenced by wide distribution over the world, must be awarded to the Terns, a very highly specialized group in structure, exaggerating the points of their more generalized family relations the Gulls. Gulls go nearly everywhere, but are much rarer in or near the Tropics than in colder regions, and rarely breed there, and are absent altogether in the Central Pacific. But you cannot go anywhere without meeting Terns, not only in remote islands, but in the interior of great continents, for there are many freshwater species. Those curious exaggerations of the Tern type, the Scissor-bills (Rhynchops), with the " under- shot " beak with which they plough the water for small fish, are all warm-climate birds, but the few species between them extend all round the world. Although there are no islands too distant for such birds as these — and of course the oceanic Petrels — to have reached, there are some which are too out-of-the-way for any land-bird. When Mr. W. L. Sclater was sent to investigate the birds of the Chagos Islands, a small group in the Indian Ocean, he found no land-bird there except the little Scarlet Weaver (Foudia madagascariensis), a most obvious human introduction ; and on Easter Island, although a small land-bird has been re- ported, it has never been brought to book, and 222 BIRD BEHAVIOUR the Tinamous found there have been introduced from Chile. In Hawaii, too, the Kingfishers and Pigeons and Parrots, which are elsewhere most widely spread in the Pacific, are wanting, though a Chinese Turtle- Dove (Turtur chinensis) has been successfully intro- duced, as also has an Australian Parrakeet, the Mealy Rosella (Platycercus pallidiceps), the original stock in the latter case being but a single pair. This being so, it is a great pity that a pair of the common Belted Kingfisher of North America (Ceryle alcyon) were shot on arrival, as they might also have found a footing, and this natural intro- duction of a new form would have been most interesting to watch. The irruption of even a large number of new birds, however, does not neces- sarily always mean success, as in the case of the failure of the well-known repeated attempts of hordes of Pallas's Sand-Grouse to colonize Western Europe, where they hatched young even with us. An interesting example of successful emigration, and that by a bird which nowadays very rarely migrates at all, is furnished by the career of the Magpie, a bird whose unique and conspicuous appearance and — where it is not persecuted — its abundance and familiarity, have always made it one of the best known of birds, as the many stories and superstitions relating to it abundantly testify. It is very doubtful if this bird was known to the ancient Greeks, for no species mentioned in their writings can be definitely identified with it, unless THE MAGPIE'S ODYSSEY 223 it was the " Lycian Daw " of Aristotle, which is not described, but merely mentioned by name among other " Daws,3' in his work on the u History of Animals." The bird the Greeks called Kiss a or Kitta, which some scholars have taken for the Magpie, was evidently the Jay, since Aristotle mentions its characteristic habit of storing away the acorns, and in enumerating the various species of Thrushes compares the Missel-Thrush to it in size, a comparison for which the Magpie would be obviously unsuitable owing to its long tail. The Pica of the Romans, from which word the French and English " Pie " is evidently derived, was also the Jay, for here again the acorn- eating habit is mentioned by Pliny, who also speaks of his Pica, as Chaucer, so many centuries later, mentions the Jay, as a bird kept for talking. The Roman naturalist also gives us a definite clue to the extension of the Magpie's range in his time, for he says that of late years a new sort of Pie (Pica) had appeared in the neighbourhood of Rome, noticeable for its varied (or pied) colouring and long tail. Many centuries later we have a definite historical record of another westerly irruption of Magpies, when about the year 1676 a flock of about a dozen is stated to have arrived in Wexford — the ancestors of the numerous Magpies which now form so noticeable a feature in the bird-life of Ireland. That they did not inhabit that island previously is known from the " Itinerary " of Fynes Morison, who expressly notices their absence in 1617. 224 BIRD BEHAVIOUR As the Magpie is found in Western America and all along the width of Asia in the north, it would seem as if it were originally an American bird which has in the course of thousands of years been moving continually westwards, although on the other hand the American birds may be a colony which crossed Bering Straits in the opposite direction to found a colony in the New World, as the Alaska Wheatears are doing, in which case we might look to Central Asia as the cradle of the Magpie race ; it stands so much alone in the Crow tribe that it has no near relatives to give us a clue. CHAPTER VIII The senses of birds — Sight and its general high development — Degree of perception of colour — Influence of colour, if any, on courtship, and the segregation of species — Perception of the colour in various kinds of food — Smell, usually poorly developed — Exceptions noted — Acuteness of hearing — Sense of touch — Taste-perceptions. THE senses of birds are nearly all well developed, though the mechanism of the sense-organs is some- times much less elaborate than in the case of mammals, an important instance of the want of correlation of structure with function. In power of sight they, as a rule, surpass all other animals, this being the dominant sense in all birds except the Apteryxes or Kiwis of New Zealand, which see very badly, not only in the day, when they are naturally at rest, but even at night, when they will walk right up to a person when at large, and show no alarm when a white handkerchief is waved at them when in captivity. There is a possibility that there are great differ- ences in the powers of distant sight of ordinary birds, but there is little or no evidence so far on this head. Macgillivray has indeed suggested in his book on British Birds that the Spariow-Hawk is 15 225 226 BIRD BEHAVIOUR short-sighted, because he had observed it does not seem to see birds in a hedge at a hundred yards' distance, and does not attack except at a range of a few yards. The obvious criticism on this, how- ever, is that the Sparrow-Hawk, like other birds of prey, does not waste time and strength in attempt- ing attacks which have no reasonable prospect of success ; its strategy is mainly one of surprises, for though capable of very great speed for a short dash, it is not suited for a long pursuit. It there- fore pays no attention to birds which it does' not consider favourably placed for attack. Fhave seen behaviour in Kites in Calcutta which exactly suggests this. The Kite has no speed at all, but can execute a very successful surprise stoop, snatching food even from a man in a most disconcerting manner. I have seen one of these birds, passing with the slow flapping flight which it employs when travelling at a low elevation of about the height of the house-tops, evidently meditating an attack on a Dabchick and her young on the Calcutta Museum tank. The thought in the Kite's mind was obvious, as it hung on its wings for a moment over the little group ; but " chip, chip," went the Dabchick — which ordinarily never bothered about Kites — beginning to " go down by the bows " in preparation for a dive, her downy young imitat- ing her, evidently reading the enemy's intentions ; and the enemy's comprehension of the hopelessness of the situation was equally complete, for the poising was but momentary, and the Kite flapped THE VISION OF VULTURES 227 on again, knowing that before its stoop could reach the water the quarry would be under it. Only occasionally were such very faint attempts made, the numerous Kites knowing full well the uselessness of attack, and this no doubt explains why they never carried off the sitting Dabchick or her young when resting on the very exposed nest. One I bought in the bazaar and threw up near the pond, failing to get under way and falling to the ground, was instantly snapped up by a Kite within a few feet of me. A dead Sparrow was also instantly picked up, though live ones were not chased ; yet a released cage-bird, if at all weak on the wing, was instantly pursued. The sight of the high-soaring birds of prey is supposed to be particularly fine, and certainly Vultures seem to be able to perceive a dead animal when themselves out of human sight, though of course they are often guided to their prey by observing the movements of others floating at a lower elevation, or of lesser scavengers such as Kites and Crows, flying near the earth. In this connection it is interesting to note that the eyes of most Vultures are small compared to those of Eagles, and are not overhung by a bony brow, perhaps because less exact observation is necessary in their case ; but in the ordinary way, size of eye seems to have no more to do with power of vision in birds than in man, the usually small- eyed Ducks and Geese being to all appearance quite as quick- sighted as any other birds. Nor does 228 BIRD BEHAVIOUR there seem to be any connection between colour of iris and visual powers, both Owls and Hawks, for instance, being found with either dark or yellow eyes ; this is, of course, not surprising, as the iris itself plays no direct part in vision, but some correlation might have conceivably existed. That some birds have powers of sight surpassing those of man is known from the use of the Great Grey Shrike by the Hawk-catchers at Valkenswaard in Holland, who are informed by their tethered Shrike of the appearance of a bird of prey before they could see it themselves, and when nearer, of its character, whether Harrier or Buzzard on the one hand, or Falcon or Goshawk, by the degree of agitation shown by the sentinel. The Indian Roller (Coracias indie a), too, sitting lazily on a telegraph wire in the glare of the Indian sun, perceives and flies down to an insect or other small prey at a distance at which human eyes would certainly not perceive such an object; while many must have wondered how a Kingfisher, or even more a Gannet, discovers a fish and is able to judge its distance from the surface sufficiently well to make a successful swoop. It must be admitted, however, that the Kingfisher at all events makes at least as many misses as hits, not having so much judgment as some of the birds of prey I have been mentioning ; and, speaking of most birds, they do not behave as if their power of sight were any better than our own. Diving-birds, however, may enjoy better sight SIGHT OF OWLS IN DAYTIME 229 under water than we do ; at any rate it is interesting to note that just as we use a water- glass to examine the bottom, so do Grebes and Cormorants often put their head under water to beyond the eyes before diving to get a clear view. These hunters under water can, however, generally see as well in the air as any ordinary birds ; in fact, those I have mentioned, and such diving-ducks as Golden-eye and Mergansers, are notoriously very wary and alert birds. Penguins, however, are suspected of being short-sighted when out of water. The powers of night vision which Owls and Nightjars possess, also, do not seem to interfere with their vision by day, the idea that they are dazzled by daylight being solely due to the fact that they are unwilling to move in the day. When they are actually on the wing, they fly well enough and avoid dangers and obstacles quite successfully, and in captivity they show interest in birds flying over, and even bask in the sun. In the case of Owls, the curious peering movements of the head certainly give the idea of bad sight, but these gestures are indulged in just as much in the dusk of the evening as in daylight, being merely a trick of habit ; indeed, something similar is seen in the more jerky head- movements of the Hawks. The blinking of the upper eyelid in Owls is also a very human action which may have helped to suggest the idea of blindness in daylight, almost all other birds winking with the inner or third eyelid, which is drawn so rapidly across the eye 23o BIRD BEHAVIOUR that it does not interfere with the gaze, and is hardly noticed by the onlooker. The Dipper, or Water-Ouzel, also blinks the upper eyelid like an Owl ; the reason of this seems obscure, since other birds that hunt under water do not do so. Nor, as far as that goes, do all Owls hunt at night ; many, such as the Snow Owl, Hawk-Owl, Short-eared Owl, and Burrowing Owl and some others, hunting either by day or by night, while the Little Owl and Indian Little Owl (Athene noctua and A. brama) come out while it is still daylight ; in India I used to notice the latter out at about five o'clock in Dehra Dun, while in Cal- cutta they did not show themselves till nightfall, no doubt owing to the hostility of the numerous Crows, for I have seen a homing Crow swoop at an Owl which had appeared a little too soon. There are also some Nightjars which hunt by day, such as the American Night-Hawk (Chordeiles popetue), which is often abroad high in the air in bright sunlight. Woodcocks and Snipe, which are usually night-birds, may also feed by day. Conversely, many of the birds which have no special adaptation for night vision often fly and feed at night, such as many members of the Duck and Plover tribes, especially the common Lapwing and Mandarin Duck, whose unusually large eyes may, however, be an adaptation to these habits. Certainly the last-named bird, in spite of his gay colours — such being almost unknown in true noc- turnal birds, which are sombre like mammals — is, PEAHEN AS ADVENTURESS 231 judging from its habits in England, quite as noc- turnal as some of the Owls, not stirring much before noon, while the trampled ground in any small enclosure in which specimens are kept shows that they run about much at night. Most Ducks, when free from human persecution, are certainly inclined to diurnal habits to a greater extent than their behaviour where sought by sports- men would indicate, and M. Roger on says that their night- vision appears to be no better than our own, judging from the way in which they will fail to see bread thrown to them at dusk. But as they generally fly in the open and feed much by feeling, this is no great drawback. The ordinary day-birds certainly are just about as much at a loss at night as we are, and can avoid danger just about as much. Game-birds are gener- ally particularly helpless at night, as we see in the case of Fowls and Pheasants ; but Peafowl are far more alert — I have known a Peahen which had escaped from the former trading menagerie at Covent Garden remain at large for months, in spite of attempts at nocturnal surprises. The Quail appears to be even actually semi-nocturnal, for in captivity it freely moves about at night ; one I kept loose in a room was quite quiescent by day. The Lineated Pheasant of Burma (Gennceus lineatus) has also been observed coming out into forest clearings on moonlight nights. Although Parrots are mostly diurnal birds, there are some as nocturnal as Owls, such as the great 232 BIRD BEHAVIOUR flightless Owl-Parrot or Kakapo of New Zealand (Stringops babroptilus), and the small, flying, but otherwise very similar Night-Parrakeet of Australia (Geopsittacus occidentalis) ; and the sheep-killing Kea seems to be active indifferently by day or by night, which is one reason why its attacks on the sheep are so hard to guard against. Lord Tavistock has also noted recently that an Australian Black Cockatoo which he had flying loose about the park at Woburn was often on the wing after dark, though of course mere flying after dark does not necessarily indicate nocturnal habits, as so many migratory day-birds fly at night. The New Zealand flightless Rails or Wekas (Ocydromus) are active by night as well as by day, and ordinary Rails are great night-fliers. Swifts are said often in hot weather to soar up and stay aloft all night. Herons often move and feed either by day or night, and Bitterns and Night-Herons (Nycticorax) are true nocturnal birds ; I noticed, however, that in our colony of the common Night-Heron (N. griseus) at Calcutta, the birds did not seem to sleep much during the day ; but the same thing can be said of most Owls, only the Barn- Owls being generally really asleep in the daytime, if one may judge from their behaviour in captivity. Petrels are nocturnal at their breeding-places, probably from fear of Gulls and Crows, for out at sea one sees them flying freely by day in the hottest sun ; I have even seen a Storm-Petrel and a Clouded Yellow butterfly flying within a few yards of each other. RECOGNITION OF COLOURS 233 Just as Macgillivray suggested the Sparrow-Hawk was short-sighted, so Professor Newton, in his " Dictionary of Birds," remarks, with reference to the easy-going way in which the Hedge- Sparrow accepts an alien egg, that for all we know to the contrary it may be colour-blind ; and within the last few years, a fancier writing to the Feathered World about Homing Pigeons has said that though they can distinguish black from white, this is not the case with colours. The possibility of some birds not seeing as we do must therefore be borne in mind, but as regards the class as a whole the general evidence certainly tends to bear out the commonly accepted idea that their vision for colour is the same as our own. This is shown in several ways ; for instance, they recognize colour in other birds, and display either friendship or hostility in consequence, according to whether they are at the time in need of com- panionship or in a position to feel jealousy. Birds which are normally spiteful, for instance, are likely to attack a species which bear colours reminiscent of their own ; thus Jenner Weir told Darwin of a case in which a Robin in an aviary killed a red- breasted Crossbill and injured a Goldfinch ; and an American Indigo-bird (Cyanospiza cyanea) at- tacked a Nonpareil (C. ciris), which has a blue head, and nearly scalped it. I have myself seen a mated Blue Australian Wren (Malurus cyaneus) in one of the Zoo aviaries furiously pursue a cock Yellow- winged Blue Sugar-bird (C&reba cyanea), 234 BIRD BEHAVIOUR which it certainly would have hunted to death had not the keeper removed it ; and the Green Cardinal (Gubernatrix cristata) has a bad name among fanciers for spitefulness to birds of similar colours. I have above commented (Chapter V) on the alarm shown by birds at the sight of young Cuckoos, though it may well be pleaded that as the pattern is the striking point here, the evidence is not necessarily in favour of colour- vision ; nor need colour-vision to be postulated to account for terror which the mixed birds in the Western Aviary at the Zoo have shown in my presence at the Grey Touracou (Schizorhis concolor), or the Parrots in the large Parrot Aviary at the dark-slate Vasa Parrot (Coracopsis vasd)> both birds of colora- tion which, although uniform, is met with in some birds of prey. I have witnessed sympathetic attraction of colour, or at any rate markings, when in Calcutta I turned a specimen of the Silver-eared Mesia (Mesia argentauris) into a large flight-cage with a mixed collection, and saw that it at first associated with the Black-capped Sibia (Malarias capistratis) and White-eared Bulbul (Molpastes leucotis), before finding out its really near relative, the Pekin Robin. The House-Crows in Calcutta used to get very excited when they saw a live or dead bird handled which showed much black or dark colour, using the same cries and gestures as they employed when seeing one of their own kind handled ; I have seen this when the subject was a Muscovy Duck, a MISPLACED GENEROSITY 235 cock Amherst Pheasant, and even a Drongo-Shrike, the sworn enemy of Crows. Here there is a case of sympathy aroused by colour, but as in the above case, no proof of perception of anything but light and dark shades. The old cock-fighters used to object to a hen- feathered cock in the pit, owing to the fact that the opponent of such a bird was likely to mistake it for a hen, and so allow it an advantage while courting it ; this might argue a perception of colour on the part of the Fowls, as the distinctive orna- ments of the cock — large comb, long hackles and sickles — were shorn before the fight, so that only the colour would be left to differentiate between the sexes. But it must be remembered that the pattern of cocks and hens is also generally very different, the upper and under surfaces of the former sex presenting marked differences in all except uniformly coloured varieties, which were rather rare in the pit game-fowl. The preying relations of birds to warningly coloured insects, to which I devoted much attention in a long series of experiments made during my residence in India, and published in the Journal of the Asiatic Society of Bengal, showed, in my opinion, distinct colour-perceptions in the species I employed for experiment, since these generally preferred more plainly coloured butterflies to the " warningly coloured " Danaids (Danais chrysippus, genutia, and limniace, and Euplcea core), and the still more striking " warningly coloured " white 236 BIRD BEHAVIOUR Delias eucharis and swallow-tail Papilio aristolochia, and of these only the two first species of Danais mentioned were at all like each other in colour, though I must admit that they could all have been distinguished by pattern also. The common Babblers I worked with appeared to be deceived by the resemblance of the female Nepberonia hippia to Danais limniace, though this is not very exact ; and two species of Drongo Shrike by the resemblance between the swallow- tail above mentioned and the mimicking form of another species (P. demoleus) which is extraordinarily like the very unpalatable aristolo chics except in having the abdomen black like most of the wings, instead of scarlet like the spots on the hinder pair of these, a distinction which was not so conspicuous in life as it sounds in print. The intelligent little Pekin Robin, however, was not to be deceived by this very close mimic, but it was taken in by the extraordinarily perfect resemblance of the female Hypolimnas misippus to Danais chrysippus, a re- semblance which is one of the " show horses " of the theory of warning coloration and mimicry, and usually deceives human entomologists if un- prepared for it. Speaking of birds' mistakes, I once saw a very amusing one made by a cock Sparrow in Calcutta, which flew down and picked up what he evidently mistook for a beetle, the creature really being a very small specimen of the local toad (Bufo melano- stictus) ; and from the movements of his bill after ONE EXPERIENCE ENOUGH 237 he had, immediately, dropped it, he certainly would have " pulled a face " had his features admitted of it. Such an instance enables one to see how very advantageous the acquisition of " warn- ing colours " might be to a nauseous animal, saving it from some experimental tasting ; but unfortu- nately for the theory, toads, and several other animals with repellent attributes, have been singu- larly unsuccessful in evolving such patterns. The mere possession of a striking pattern of the con- ventional " warning " type does not prevent experi- mental tasting by novices ; my birds often tried an insect they afterwards refused, and a very interesting case of this occurred with a Starling (Sturnus vulgaris menzbieri), which is only a winter migrant to India, and probably knows little of the taste of Indian butterflies. I offered this bird one of the black-and-scarlet swallow-tails above-mentioned, which was immediately gulped whole with charac- teristic Starling greediness. But next day another was not even touched, at which I was not surprised, as this butterfly is so objectionable to birds that often they will not even kill it. So the lesson was well learnt, and such lessons may be long remembered ; to a Starling I kept in England, which, when I bought it, had been kept for months in close captivity, and had had no chance of seeing caterpillars, I offered those of the buff-tip moth (Pygara bucepbala), well known to be un- palatable to birds, and distinguished by a striking black- and-yellow chequered coloration ; the bird 238 BIRD BEHAVIOUR simply turned them over with its bill in search of something better beneath, as if they had been so many ends of string or bits of stick, while when I had offered it a much larger green caterpillar on a twig it ravenously tore it from its hold and devoured it. This experiment, by the way, wants repeating under circumstances fairer to the caterpillar, though as a matter of fact I was not trying to find out if the bird would miss the insect owing to its protective coloration, as I have once seen a green lizard do. That is to say, if the power of a bird to see through a protective disguise is to be tested, the insects should be in situ beforehand in .a place to which the bird is admitted without having been allowed to see the preparations and so have its expectations raised. Until it is experimentally proved that birds overlook insects of protective appearance in their natural environment, we are not justified in saying that such appearance is of any survival value as far as feathered enemies are concerned. In the case of birds of prey there happens to be some evidence of the value of protective resemblance; a tame Peregrine Falcon flown at a Houbara Bus- tard (Houbara undulata) has been seen, when the quarry had settled and squatted, to alight and search all round on foot without success, and Lloyd, an excellent observer, says in his " Wild Sports in Sweden " that the Swedes when trapping for Hawks use a light Fowl, Pigeon, or what not for bait in ordinary weather, but a dark one if NO COLOUR-LINE OBSERVED 239 snow is on the ground. Moreover,Mr. J. G. Millais in his " Breath from the Veldt " describes how the Bateleur Eagle (Helotarsus ecaudatus) often captures prey owing to its habit of scanning the ground behind as well as before it as it soars, other Eagles passing straight on and missing prey which has squatted on their approach, only to get up and move directly they have passed. In all these cases, however, we must remember that immobility may be a more important factor than colour. With regard to the fruit food of birds, it is to be noted that they eat red currants sooner than the white variety of that fruit, but colour may not decide this. Whatever attention birds pay to the colour and pattern of their prey or foes, they seem singularly inattentive to it as regards recognizing their own racial affinities, for the theory of recognition-marks, which supposes that species of birds which are closely alike except for colour and pattern are aided in selecting partners by such differences, is not borne out by facts. Whenever the difference between two forms is so slight that colour, not structure or note, is the only distinction, the birds themselves disregard it, no matter how glaring the difference may be. This is best known in the case of the Hooded Crow and Carrion Crow (Corvus comix and C. cor one) in the case of our own fauna ; these birds seldom breed in the same district, but when they do they frequently cross, and the same is the case with these 240 BIRD BEHAVIOUR species where they meet in their breeding-range outside Britain, as in Central Siberia. Here also our Goldfinch breeds and interbreeds with the Grey-headed Goldfinch of the Himalayas (Carduelis caniceps), a bird provided with excellent recognition marks. Actual specimens illustrating these points can be seen in the Entrance Hall at the South Kensington Museum. American naturalists will recall the case of the Red-shafted and Yellow-shafted Flickers (Colaptes cafer and C. auratus), two inter-breeding Wood- peckers also well distinguished by hues and patterns, and Anglo-Indians that of the Indian and Burmese Rollers or " Blue- jays " (Coracias indica and C. affinis), the latter much darker than the former, and devoid of its conspicuous terminal tail-bar, yet producing numerous intergrades, while the pure forms, as in the case of the Crows, have actually been seen paired up together. Many similar cases might be cited ; in fact, just where " recognition-marks" might be expected to be of service, there they uniformly prove inoperative to segregate species — as might indeed have been expected when we see our variable domestic birds infallibly recognize their own species in spite of abnormalities in colour. Neither does the study of the courtship of birds exhibiting colour-variations encourage the idea of preference for a particular type of colour. Sir Ralph Heron recorded, nearly a century ago, how all his Peahens fell in love with a pied Peacock, and DISFIGUREMENT NO BAR 241 Mr. D. Dewar has observed that white Peacocks in the Lahore Zoo had superior charms to the coloured birds ; while I have seen London park Mallard Ducks not only pair with all-white, grey- breasted, and rufous-flanked drakes in preference to the typical male in its full beauty of colouring, but in one case recently even with a Spotted-bill Drake (Anas 'pcecilorhyncha), which has no distinctive male plumage at all. M. Roger on also says that this Indian species, the African Yellow-bill (A. undulata) and Australian Wild Duck (A. super ciliosa), none of which have any distinct sex-coloration, never- theless interbreed with Mallard as if all were of one species. Recently I saw at Kew a Mandarin Drake whose left eye had been destroyed, and whose face on that side was abnormally white, paired with a fine unpinioned female, in spite of the presence of perfect drakes, one at least unpinioned, which I saw her charge with tail wagging defiantly, instead of inciting her mate to do it, as is commonly done by this most affectionate and selective species. Another bird at the Zoo with misplaced wing, spoil- ing his colour-scheme entirely, also has had a mate for years, though Mandarin Ducks, like Mallard, will tolerate bigamy rather than take to a drake they dislike. I have, however, seen a case in the Calcutta Zoo in which a Mandarin Drake of superior plumage was preferred by the ducks to others, though these were not positively defective ; and I have found also in Calcutta a Linnet hen prefer a lame male with a 16 242 BIRD BEHAVIOUR richer-coloured breast to a poorer-hued but per- fect bird, and a hen Avadavat (Estrelda amandava) prefer in two cases a bright to a duller-coloured male. If birds have the aesthetic sense with which Darwin credited them, they may, like us, think more of small differences than large ones ; golden hair is generally admired by man, but no good judge would put it, as a point of beauty, above well-chiselled features, though it catches the eye more at first. As against the possibility of birds generally being colour-blind, we may perhaps set the preference of the Australian Satin Bower-bird (Ptilonorhyncbus holosericeus) for blue when choosing decorations for its bower, and the liking of some Weaver-birds for green and yellow wool when given this substance to amuse themselves with in a cage. It will be noted that yellow is a common colour among Weavers, and that the eyes of the above- mentioned Bower-bird are blue, a very rare colour in live birds' eyes, though taxidermists are fond of it in glass ones ! Before leaving the subject of the sense of sight in birds, attention must be drawn to its extra- ordinary acuteness in Humming-birds, which, as I have been able to observe in the case of captive birds, appear to be able to see glass ; at any rate they do not fly against it as other birds constantly do. The sense of smell in birds seems to be little developed as a rule, not more than in ourselves, at all events judging by their behaviour. DEVELOPMENT OF SCENT 243 The Apteryxes, however, are exceptions, as these birds, which unlike all others have the nostrils at the tip of the beak, nose their way about like a beast, even sniffing audibly. Some sportsmen also are of opinion that wildfowl have a keen scent, and should be approached accordingly with due regard to the direction of the wind, as in stalking deer and other similar animals ; and decoymen used to burn a turf before their mouths when working a decoy, so as to hide the human scent. St. John also recorded that his domesticated wild Ducks scented out a heap of diseased potatoes which had been well covered with earth. On the other hand, Mr. Millais found that wild Geese approached quite near him when concealed in a pit, so as to suggest they had no particular power of scent. The sense may be well developed in the Crows, for M. Rogeron says a pet Jackdaw of his could distinguish between salt and powdered sugar, which nevertheless were alike to the eye, taking only a few grains of the one substance and a big beakful of the other, and Dickens describes how his second tame Raven disinterred the halfpence and bits of cheese his , predecessor had buried in the garden. Pigeons also used at any rate to be credited by fanciers with liking the smell of aniseed, which was supposed to attach them to a cote. The only case in which I myself have seen any- thing which suggested scenting power in a bird was that of a Pied Hornbill (Anthracoceros mala- baricus) I kept in India, which, when offered 244 BIRD BEHAVIOUR butterflies, refused the Danaids after pinching them with the tip of the bill, and treated a cigar- end in the same way. The latter substance might have been rejected by touch, but in the case of the insects, as the bird ate other butterflies, it seems only natural to conclude that it detected an objectionable scent by means of the posterior nares ; taste was out of the question, as the tongue in the Hornbills is so very short, and the beak, for some inches from the tip, as dry and horny inside as out. With regard to the very birds in which one would expect scenting powers to be particularly well developed, the Vultures, all the evidence is against this, those who have experience unanimously declaring that a carcase if well covered over is not detected by them ; and in Darwin's classical experi- ment with a hungry Condor the bird did not appreciate the nature of the contents of a paper parcel of fresh meat till he touched it with his beak, in which close proximity the odour of raw flesh would no doubt be detected by a human nose. In spite of the less elaborate structure of the internal ear, and of the absence of an external one altogether, there is no doubt that the hearing of birds is at least as good as our own, and in many cases possibly even better. This is particularly remarkable, because not only is the outer ear or auricle wanting, but the ear-hole itself is generally overhung by a dense patch of short and very firmly rooted feathers, the ear-coverts, which sometimes remain when the rest of the head is naked, as in the GEESE AS NIGHT-WATCHMEN 245 Sarus and Australian Cranes (Antigone collaris and A. australasiana). In most bald birds, however, the ear-hole is exposed, as in the Ostrich and Turkey, and so it is, curiously enough, in an Australian bird with otherwise feathered head, the Mallee-hen (Leipoa ocellata). The perfection of birds' hearing may be accur- ately judged of by the performances of the various talking and mimicking birds, such as Parrots and Mynahs, whose imitation of their models is often absolutely perfect ; and the susceptibility to sound of species which have not these vocal gifts is well known. It is said that the wild Turkey, when being lured within shot by an imitation of the hen's call, will at once detect a false note, and be shy for the rest of the season ; and the Canadian Goose dis- tinguishes at once, according to Audubon, between man-made sounds and the natural ones of the woods and wilds, the crack of a dry stick under a deer's hoof being discriminated from its breakage by a human foot, and the accidental slap of £ paddle against a canoe-side from the flop of a Turtle taking to the water. Every one also is familiar with the Roman Geese whose vigilance is said to have saved the Capitol ; and whatever the historical value of the human part of that portion of Roman history, no goose- breeder, says Fowler, as quoted in Wright's " Book of Poultry," would at all doubt it, his own experi- ence corroborating it in every essential point. The Roman poet's attribution of greater sagacity to 246 BIRD BEHAVIOUR Geese than to Dogs is also borne out by a corre- spondent of Hewitt's quoted in the same work, who characterized Spanish (Chinese) Geese and Guinea-Fowls, kept in a lonely place, as the best watch- dogs in the neighbourhood, the actual dogs themselves generally only giving the second warning, so that it was thought they themselves relied on the birds. The Peacock is also not inferior to the Guinea-Fowl in wariness at night, and probably also relies on hearing ; a few Pea-fowl in game- coverts might prove an excellent and inexpensive guard against poachers. It has recently been noted in the press, too, that Parrots kept in fortresses and on the Eiffel Tower during the war gave warning of approaching aeroplanes when they could not possibly have seen them, and two writers, one who kept a Sulphur- crested Cockatoo and the other a Ring-necked Parrakeet, have recorded, the former in " Notes on Cage-birds " and the latter in the Avicultural Magazine, that the birds became aware of their masters' arrival at home at a distance at which any hearing seemed impossible. Even the Kiwi will jump if one claps one's hands suddenly close to it at night, though the waving of a white handkerchief fails to impress its dull sight. The sense of hearing is much more important in determining the relations of birds to each other than is generally supposed, and appears to be more important than sight. Thus, the freely interbreeding species which have been above RECOGNITION OF RELATIVES 247 commented on resemble each other in voice, even when this differs greatly in the two sexes, as in the Ducks of the genus Anas mentioned, and birds at once recognize and respond to a sufficiently accurate reproduction of their call, as all field-naturalists are well aware. Thus, I have made both a wild jungle-cock and tame roosters answer me, though I am unable to imitate more delicate notes, and have made wild Duck circle round and lower their flight by quacking to them. People who do not want vicious cocks, by the way, should carefully refrain from mocking their birds thus, as when the fowl's brain has grasped the location of the chal- lenge in the human, he will attack, and remain permanently spiteful, in which case he is dangerous to small children. Different species — too far apart to readily inter- breed— will associate if the note is similar, as in the case of the Whistling Tree-Ducks (Dendrocycna), when specimens find themselves isolated in cap- tivity, different though they may be in colour. But allied species have a wonderful knack of know- ing each other by sight, even if colour and voice are both different ; thus, the Mandarin and Caro- lina drakes not only occasionally make love to each other's females, which is not surprising, as these are so much alike that one generally has to look at them twice to see which is which, but the females will reciprocate, although the two male birds are very distinct both in coloration, decorative plumage, and voice. 248 BIRD BEHAVIOUR I was much amused on a most miserable Sunday one January to see a very agitated Mandarin Duck which had got left out in the pairing which was going on despite the cold rain, establishing an understanding with an equally forlorn Carolina drake, in spite of differences of language. Her gestures made it clear to him that she wanted him to drive off Mandarin drakes who happened to pass, and I saw him get two duckings from these while I watched, 'although she was quite ready her- self to follow any mated Mandarin till he turned to drive her off. The note of the male of her species certainly seems to have a very potent effect on the hen at times ; twice I have seen the common Starling fly to her mate when singing, and pairing take place, the only occasions on which I have observed this, and possibly the " instrumental music " practised by some birds may have charms to soothe the feminine breast. On the only occasion on which I ever saw the pairing of the Green Peafowl (Pavo muticus), the hen signified her assent just after he had rustled his train for the second time, although on the first rustling, after she had apparently been admiring his display with unusual attention, she slipped round behind him, only to come forward again. At such times both this and the common Peafowl utter a long-drawn and peculiarly shrill scream, quite unlike their usual call, and this may have its effect on the hen. I have also seen the common VANDALISM IN SONG-BIRDS 249 Collared Dove stoop to her mate when he was cooing, and the House- Mynah in India similarly respond to the chattering song and bowing move- ments of the male, though I have also seen her turn on him for displaying, like the hen Sparrow. In spite of their powers of hearing being similar in character to our own, and their voices often pleasing us, birds have no taste in music, as we understand it, though we must also remember that savage or primitive music does not appeal to us, nor do we as adults enjoy the horrible noises, such as slate-pencils " scrooped " on slates, which seem to please children. But it is at least curious that birds are so wilfully perverse, as we should call it, in their selection of sounds to respond to and imitate ; a Canary can be got to sing in answer to the working of a sewing- machine or the fizzling of a frying-pan, and will often spoil its song by the interpolation of a Spar- row's chatter ; while the various mocking species of birds, such as the true Mocking-bird of North America (Mimus polyglottus) and the Shama of India (Cittocincla macrura) persistently degrade their beautiful songs by imitating harsh cries. I have also heard the Indian Orange-headed Ground- Thrush (Geocichla citrina), a species which combines the excellences of the Song-Thrush and Blackbird, irritatingly repeat a most trivial and monotonous note it had picked up in the Zoo aviaries. Some birds seem to be able to hear notes in- audible to us ; thus, the Starling when singing 250 BIRD BEHAVIOUR frequently opens and shuts its bill without pro- ducing a sound that I can hear, at any rate, but no doubt the hen hears it ; and some of the little Eastern Weaver-Finches known as Nuns or Manni- pais (Munia), go through all the gestures of singing while emitting scarcely any noise audible to us but a faint mew at the finish. As the hens listen very attentively, they no doubt can hear, and evidently like the music, such as it is, though even to them it cannot sound very loud, judging from their close attention. The sense of touch in birds is certainly not facilitated by their structure — generally horny beaks and scale-cased feet, and feather-covered body ; but it is reasonably acute nevertheless. In Ducks, in which the beak is covered, except at the tip and edges, with skin instead of horn, it is no doubt more acute than in most, and the common Sheldrake has the expanded edges of the beak near the tip so soft that it can fairly be said to have lips, while in the Australian Pink-eyed Duck (Malacorhynchus membranaceus) these lips are quite large and hang down like the flews of a hound, so that the beak should have much tactile sensi- bility. In Snipe and Woodcock, also, the bill is quite soft at the tip, and these birds, like Ducks, feed by feeling in mud ; but hard-billed waders like Storks also grope in mud with much success, and many of these birds also feel in the mud with their feet, though this, like the scratching action often per- THE PROBLEM OF WHISKERS 251 formed by Ducks in the water, has no doubt for its primary object the loosening of the bottom to stir up lurking prey. The cere, or soft skin at the base of the beak in Parrots, birds of prey, and Pigeons, may have some tactile value, preventing these birds from plunging the beak over the nostrils in the soft food, flesh or fruit, on which so many of these birds subsist ; at any rate, the only other groups in which the beak has a soft covering are the Ducks and Flamin- goes, in which only the end and edges, as has been said, are horny, and these are habitually mud- feeders, and seek food by feeling. The soft flanges or lips, at the base of the bill in the nestlings of Passerines, Hoopoes, and Woodpeckers, are also sensitive to touch, and no doubt aid the young "yellow-beak" to perceive the food the parents offer it ; but they are not found in other young birds even of the helpless kinds, and never in active chicks. Whiskers like those of many mammals are found round the face and base of the bill in the Apteryx or Kiwi, so beast-like in its behaviour, and pre- sumably they may serve as feeling- organs in the same way ; but it is less easy to divine the use of the bristles found at the sides of the beak, and often where it joins the forehead, in many birds which feed on flying insects, and specially conspicu- ous in the Nightjars and Flycatchers. They may act as guides for the snap of the beak, but they are absent in many birds of somewhat similar habits 252 BIRD BEHAVIOUR to those which possess them ; for instance, the Nightjars of the genus Chordeiles ;have none, nor do the Swallows or Swallow- Shrikes exhibit them. Yet they are particularly well developed in the Barbets of the genus XantholNDON AND AYIJCSBURY. BIOLOGY LIBRARY .,9 9^31 3503 Life Sciences Bida. 64^-25 Ji. ALL BOOKS MAY BE RECALLED AFTER 7 DAYS Renewed books are subject to immediate DUE AS STAMPED BELOW UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA, BERKELEY FORM NO. DD4, 7m, 477 BERKELEY, CA 94720 LIBRARY UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA LIBRARY