• / . i •:•• • •v • - • - , ?k:- -•• ; • - . 1 1 -.-. - i • ,.•_.. K 1 •.: -... . - , ..-..- ' • • m m 1 m 1 ;-. :-• --•- /••• :'-•• M '•- ' •--. •: -.• • i •;-.•-.••. . ! ..:;:r''- '.':- •:• m •• -•'-..-- -;;•';:"••-;,-: Selection and Cross-breeding in Relation to the Inheritance of Coat-pigments and Coat-patterns in Rats and Guinea-pigs BY HANSFORD MAcCURDY AND W. E. CASTLE WASHINGTON, D. C. ! PUBLISHED BY THE CARNEGIE INSTITUTION OF WASHINGTON MAY, 1907 CARNEGIE INSTITUTION OF WASHINGTON PUBLICATION No. 70. PAPERS OF THE STATION FOR EXPERIMENTAL EVOLUTION, No. 8. CONTRIBUTIONS FROM THE ZOOLOGICAL LABORATORY OF THE MUSEUM OF COMPARATIVE ZOOLOGY AT HARVARD COLLEGE. E. L. MARK, DIRECTOR. No. iSS. 3 PRESS OF GIBSON BROS. WASHINGTON, D. C. CONTENTS. Page. Continuous versus discontinuous variations as factors in evolution .... i Variations of rats in coat-color and coat-pattern 4 Mendelian inheritance of coat-colors and coat-patterns in rats 7 Albinism recessive in relation to all types of pigmentation 7 Latent pigment characters and coat-patterns 8 Modification of hooded pattern by crossing with Irish 1 1 Modification of hooded pattern by selection 14 Selection for reduced stripe 14 Selection for stripe of increased size 17 Individual spots of guinea-pigs not unit characters 18 Color-patterns selected 18 Dutch-marked series (Series D) . 20 Head-spot series (Series //) 23 Statistical analysis of the data for Series D and // 25 Nose-spot series (Series N} 26 Conclusions 32 Tables 34 Bibliography .... . . 50 in SELECTION AND CROSS-BREEDING IN RELATION TO THE INHERITANCE OF COAT-PIGMENTS AND COAT- PATTERNS IN RATS AND GUINEA-PIGS. BY HANSFORD MACCURDY AND W. E. CASTLE. CONTINUOUS VERSUS DISCONTINUOUS VARIATIONS AS FACTORS IN EVOLUTION. It is generally agreed that the course of evolution is largely influenced by two factors, variation and heredity; but opinions differ as to what sorts of variation have evolutionary significance and as to the manner of their inheritance. It has been recognized by several investigators that variations are of two distinct sorts. Bateson has called these two sorts of variation contin- uous and discontinuous; more recently De Vries has called them fluctuations and mutations, respectively. By continuous variation (or fluctuation) we understand ordinary indi- vidual variation within a species. The individuals differ among themselves in size, color, and other structural features. By examining a considerable number of them we can form an idea of what is the commonest (or modal) condition as regards each structural feature ; and likewise what is the average (or mean) condition. Usually, but not always, the modal and mean conditions are approximately the same, and any other condition is the less frequent in occurrence, the greater its deviation from them. It follows that the most extreme condi- tion observed is connected with the most usual (or modal) condition by an unbroken series of intermediate conditions, and we may call the series as a whole "continuous." The distribution of the individuals in such a series is governed by the laws of "chance," and may be successfully analyzed by statistical methods. We commonly think of a "chance" result as something entirely beyond the control of law, but in reality such is not the case. Nothing is beyond the control of law. If a blindfolded person puts his hand into an urn con- taining a mixture of black and of white balls, it is a matter of chance whether he grasps a black or a white ball; but if he repeats the operation a con- siderable number of times, it is perfectly certain that he will draw balls of 2 INHERITANCE OF COAT-PIGMENTS AND COAT-PATTERNS both sorts in approximately the same proportions in which they occur in the jar. The result is a "chance" one, but controlled by a perfectly definite mathematical law. A "chance result" has been aptly defined as the result of a number of causes acting independently of each other. If this is a valid definition, then a continuous series of variations is due to no single cause but to several mutually independent ones. Some of the causes may be external in origin, others internal; some temporary in their action, others permanent. It should not surprise us, therefore, to find that continuous variations differ greatly in the degree of their inheritance. De Vries, indeed, has maintained that they are not inherited at all, except temporarily; that selection of abmodal variations from a continuous series is unable permanently to modify a race ; that the modifications will persist only so long as selection continues, but will speedily disappear when selection is arrested. This conclusion, however, seems to us altogether too sweeping. A priori there is no reason to suppose that all the causes operative to produce continuous variation are external in origin and temporary in action, as De Vries's conclusion would seem to imply. If there are in operation, in the production of a continuous series of variations, causes internal in origin, resident in the constitution of the germinal substance, so much of the result as is due to those causes should be inherited and so should be permanent. De Vries, we believe, has overlooked this factor entering into the problem. He has assumed that all the causes of continuous variation (''fluctuations") are either external in origin or due to conditions of the germinal substance purely temporary. He holds, we believe rightly, that all inheritance is due to germinal modification; but assumes, we believe without sufficient warrant, that permanent germinal modification is not a factor in the production of fluctuations. Another category of variations, discontinuous variations (which include the mutations of De Vries), is considered by Bateson and De Vries as the true and only expression of permanent germinal modification. But, grant- ing the truly germinal origin of mutations, it does not follow that they are the only product of germinal modification. A discontinuous variation, as the name suggests, is unconnected by inter- mediate conditions with the usual (modal) condition of the species. It represents a change, more or less abrupt, from the modal condition of the species, and is strongly inherited, a fact which indicates clearly its exclu- sively germinal origin. In the category of discontinuous variations belong abrupt changes in pigmentation and hairiness among both animals and plants, changes in the number of digits or of the number of phalanges in a digit among vertebrates, in the presence or absence of horns among animals and spines among plants, and other similar conditions. IN RATS AND GUINEA-PIGS. 3 Such changes are not the result of selection; they often appear, as it seems, spontaneously, and they are permanent in the race, if isolated. De Vries maintains that all species-forming variations are of this sort; that selection is unable to form new species, because it can neither call into existence mutations nor permanently modify a race by cumulation of abmodal fluctuations. Darwin, on the other hand, and the great majority of his followers, while admitting that races are occasionally produced by discontinuous or "sport" variation, ascribe evolutionary progress chiefly to the cumulation through long periods of time of slight individual differences, such as De Vries calls fluctuations. The issue between the two views is sharp and clear. According to De Vries, if we rightly understand him, selection is not a factor in the production of new species, but only in their perpetuation, since it determines merely what species shall survive; accord- ing to the Darwinian view, new species arise through the direct agency of selection, which leads to the cumulation of fluctuating variations of a particular sort. De Vries and the Darwinians differ not only as to the part which selection plays in evolution, but also as to the nature of the material upon which selection acts. According to De Vries, species are not modified by selection ; mutations are new species and selection determines only what mutations shall survive, fluctuations having no evolutionary significance. On the Darwinian view, all species, whether arising by mutation or not, are subject to modification by selection. A great deal can be said in favor of each of these contrasted views, but dis- cussion is at present less needed than experimental tests of the views outlined. To De Vries we owe much for showing that such tests are possible. It was our purpose to make tests of this sort when we undertook the experiments described in this paper. The questions to which principally attention has been directed are these: (i) Can discontinuous variations be modified by selection alone? (2) Can discontinuous variations be modified by cross-breeding? A negative answer to these questions will support the view of De Vries; an affirmative answer will support the Darwinian view, because it will show that through selection new conditions of organic stability can be obtained; that is, new species may be produced. The material used consisted of certain discontinuous variations in the color-pattern of rats. The general result obtained is this: Various color- patterns, like the several pigments found in the rodent coat, are mutually alternative in heredity. Each group of individuals referred to the same type of color-pattern forms a continuous series fluctuating in accordance with the laws of chance about a common modal condition. The different types in general do not overlap ; they form a discontinuous series. Now, these types may be modified in two different ways : (a) By selection of abmodal 4 INHERITANCE OF COAT-PIGMENTS AND COAT-PATTERNS variates within the same continuous series, and (6) by cross-breeding between different types. There is no evidence that one of these methods has effects less permanent than the other. So far, then, as these experiments go, they support the Darwinian view rather than that of De Vries. VARIATIONS OF RATS IN COAT-COLOR AND COAT-PATTERN. Variations in the pigmented coat of rodents are of two principal sorts: (i) Variations in the character of the pigments found in the coat, and (2) variations in the distribution of those pigments. The character of the pigmentation in the wild rodent is nearly always complex. Two or three pigments are associated together in the same hair, but they differ in their regional distribution, so that a grayish or brown "ticked" coat results, incon- spicuous against many backgrounds. The coat of the house-mouse (Bate- son, 103) and that of the wild guinea-pig (Castle, 105) contain three optically different pigments — yellow, brown, and black. These all coexist in the same hair. In certain fancy varieties of these rodents, a single pigment is present without the others, or the distribution of the pigments is such that only one sort is conspicuous. Animals pigmented thus are known as black, chocolate, and yellow (or red) varieties. If all three pigments are absent from the coat and likewise from the retina of the eye, a condition known as total albinism obtains. In rats, rabbits, and certain other rodents, black and yellow self-varieties are well known, but no pure chocolate animal has been observed. Total albinism and the several "self" conditions of pigmentation are all mutually alternative in inheritance. Variations in pigment distribution on the body result commonly either from entire absence of pigment from certain regions of the body, in which case the coat has white markings, or from the occurrence of different pig- ments singly in different body-regions, in which case the body bears spots of different colors. Both sorts of variation may occur simultaneously, in which case the body is spotted with pigments of different sorts and with white. The color-varieties of rats are fewer and simpler than those of mice, rab- bits, and guinea-pigs. Aside from albinos, there are only two "self" (i. e., uniformly colored) varieties, namely, gray (or brown, the color of the wild Mus decumanus) and black. Gray is a Mendelian dominant in relation to black. As regards coat-pattern, there occur two conditions of partial albinism, which differ from each other only in degree, but which may be obtained each probably in a pure (homozygous) condition. These two patterns may be called "Irish" and "hooded." Each occurs either with gray or with black pigmentation. The "Irish" of fanciers, as described by Doncaster (:o6), PLATE 1 - *••••• 1 ••••••I ••••••I !•••••» •••••••it ••••••••Itafc, •••••••••3lt •••••••••••• •••••••••••a ••••••••••I* •••••••••••• •••••••••••it !••!!!•«•••& •••••••• i-sKssnsr i-issiussi PI !•••••• ::::=;" ::::":::s ::::::::Ei;:»J;»ss:g» ••••••••••• ••"'/S • ••••••••••••••• VARIATION IN EXTENT OF PIGMENTATION AMONG CROSS-BRED HOODED RATS. The usual condition in the original stock is shown in Fig. 4, reduced pigmen- tation in Figs. I and 3, and increased pigmentation in Fig. 2. IN RATS AND GUINEA-PIGS. 5 applies only to black-pigmented individuals, but for lack of a better descrip- tive term, we shall apply it to all animals having the color-pattern of "Irish" rats, whether gray or black pigmented. A rat of the Irish pattern has pigmented sides and dorsal surface, but bears more or less white fur upon its belly, varying in extent from a few white hairs midway between the front legs to a wholly white ventral surface. Doncaster (:o6) has distinguished two types of Irish rats, in one of which the white is more extensive than in the other. He has observed that the rats with larger white areas are regularly heterozygous, producing hooded as well as Irish offspring when mated inter se. Our experiments in the main confirm this idea. It is not possible to determine with certainty, from the size of the ventral patch alone, whether a particular Irish rat does or does not contain the hooded pattern in a recessive condition, but in a lot of Irish rats of similar ancestry those with the larger white patch oftener transmit the hooded condition, while those which do not transmit the hooded condi- tion oftener have a small white patch. In hooded rats (pi. i, figs. 1-4) the white areas are more extensive than in Irish ones; pigment occurs only on the head, shoulders, and forelegs (con- stituting the "hood" of fanciers), and as a median dorsal stripe extending back on to the tail, the stripe being sometimes continuous, sometimes inter- rupted, and of varying width. According to the nature of the pigment which they bear, hooded rats may be distinguished as gray hooded or as black hooded, precisely as animals bearing the Irish pattern are designated either gray Irish or black Irish. When crosses are made between rats differing in color-pattern, the more extensively pigmented pattern tends to dominate in the offspring, a fact recognized by Crampe ('77-84, '85), Bateson (103) and Doncaster (:o6). The dominance, however, is not complete, so that the result might be de- scribed as "goneoclinic," *'. e., intermediate between the parental forms but approximating one much more nearly than the other, in this case always the more heavily pigmented one. Thus, a cross between a wild gray male rat and a black hooded female, known to be homozygous, produced a litter of seven young, all gray, but with a small patch of white on the chest, vary- ing in extent from merely a few white hairs to an area of 4 to 5 sq. cm. The same wild male, mated with an albino female, produced a litter of young similar in character, all with some white below. Bateson (103, p. 78, foot- note) mentions a similar result obtained by Miss Douglas. Again, a cross between an Irish and a hooded individual produces Irish offspring. An example will be found on page n in the matings of black Irish 9 141 with black-hooded males, producing nineteen offspring, all Irish. Most matings of Irish with hooded rats have in our experiments produced offspring of both sorts, not because of reversal in the nature of the domi- 6 INHERITANCE OP COAT-PIGMENTS AND COAT-PATTERNS nance, but because the majority of the Irish rats used happen to have been heterozygous, bearing the hooded pattern as a recessive character; but, as we have seen, this is not a necessary relationship. Hooded rats, however, bear no other color-pattern, so that any pair of hooded rats of whatever pedigree will produce only hooded pigmented offspring. To sum up in a brief statement the principal facts presented concerning the coat-patterns of rats, the following series of conditions may be recog- nized, each with pigmentation less extensive than the preceding: (i) Self, whole body pigmented; (2) Irish, whole body pigmented except more or less of the ventral surface; (3) Hooded, only the head, shoulders, and usually a median dorsal stripe pigmented; (4) Albino, no pigmentation. Each condition behaves as a dominant toward those which follow it in the series, and as a recessive toward those which precede it. Bateson (103, p. 81) refers to Crampe's failure to obtain the self or the albino conditions by selection from the parti-colored, and adds : "The types are in fact definite, and can not be built up by cumulative selection." The statement applies strictly only to the two extremes of the series, viz, self and albino, but by implication to the others also. Our experiments, however, indicate that it is possible to modify by selection and cross-breeding both the Irish and the hooded conditions, leading to the production of intermediate conditions. We suspect that the same may be true of the self condition also. It has been pointed out elsewhere (Castle and Forbes, :o6; Castle, :o6), that the theoretical absolute gametic purity, in Mendelian inheritance, probably does not exist in any case. Heterozygosis leads inevitably to modification in character of the conjugating gametes, not simply as regards the entire assemblage of characters, but as regards each character considered sepa- rately. The degree of modification is probably indicated roughly by the imperfection of dominance in the heterozygote. Thus, when a perfect blend is obtained, as in the inheritance of ear-length in rabbits, the gametes transmit that intermediate character. But when dominance is very com- plete, as in a cross between a self and an albino mammal, the segregation of self and albino conditions is very complete among the gametes formed by the cross-breeds. Yet when spotted individuals result from cross-breeding between self and albinos, these spotted individuals form gametes which transmit that same mosaic character. Now, the cross between self and albino rats gives rats of Irish pattern, but quite variable. But our experi- ments indicate that from variable Irish rats one can by selection obtain rats transmitting no pattern but Irish. The theoretical importance of this is obvious. Cross-breeding and selection combined are means by which we may not only modify existing Mendelian characters, but may even create new ones. They are, then, factors of prime importance in evolution, even in the case of characters which vary discontinuously. IN RATS AND GUINEA-PIGS. 7 MENDELIAN INHERITANCE OF COAT-COLORS AND COAT-PATTERNS IN RATS. While pigment character and color-pattern are both inherited in Mendelian fashion, the two are entirely independent of each other. They are separate and uncorrelated unit characters. Accordingly we find that each type of pigmented coat-pattern, namely, self, Irish, and hooded, may occur either with gray or with black pigmentation, the frequencies with which they occur in the respective combinations being governed by the laws of chance and of Mendelian dominance. This will appear in the detailed discussion of the experiments. Not only may each coat-pattern occur either in a gray or in a black pigmented individual, but it may occur also in an unpigmented animal. Para- doxical as this statement may seem, it is capable of abundant proof. The coat-pattern of course is not visible in an unpigmented (albino) rat, but its presence there as a potentiality can be demonstrated as certainly as the occurrence of a recessive character in a heterozygous dominant individual. Nothing but the presence of pigment is necessary to make the color-pattern manifest. This can be supplied by a mating with a pigmented animal. Specific pigment potentialities (gray, black, or both) are likewise present in albinos. Consequently we must recognize that albinos transmit inactive both pigments and color-patterns; these, however, are unseen and can not be made visible until some lacking substance borne by all pigmented indi- viduals is supplied. Characters transmitted in this inactive state have been termed by one of us (Castle, : 05) latent, and that terminology will be followed in the present paper. ALBINISM RECESSIVE IN RELATION TO ALL TYPES OF PIGMENTATION. That total albinism behaves as a recessive Mendelian character has been recognized independently by a number of investigators, among the earliest being Correns (: 01) and Cuenot (: 02). The fact has been abundantly veri- fied in the case of mice (see Castle and Allen, 103), rats (Crampe, '77-84; Bateson, 103; Doncaster, :o6), rabbits (Woods, 103; Hurst :o5; and Castle 105), and guinea-pigs (Castle, 105). The experiments described in this paper corroborate those of Crampe and of Doncaster with reference to rats. The proportions of albinos and of pigmented individuals in mixed litters are close to the Mendelian expectations, indicating neither selective union of gametes nor lessened fertility of certain sorts of unions. Pigmented rats, in which albinism was recessive, when mated inter se, have produced 129 albinos to 384 pigmented young, the numbers expected being 128 and 385, respectively. Albinos mated with pigmented individuals, in which albinism 8 INHERITANCE OP COAT-PIGMENTS AND COAT-PATTERNS was recessive, have produced 201 albinos to 244 pigmented young, or 45.1 per cent albinos, where 50 per cent are expected. The deviation of nearly 5 per cent from expectation in the latter case is probably a matter of chance and would grow less with more extensive observations, for in studying the inheritance of total albinism some observers record an excess of albinos (Castle, 105), others an excess of pigmented individuals (Allen, 104), while others observe very close agreement with expectation (Cu^not, Darbishire). This is as we should expect on the theory of probabilities (see Allen, 104, p. 82). LATENT PIGMENT CHARACTERS AND CO AT- PATTERNS. Latent transmission of pigment characters through albinos is a matter requiring fuller consideration. Assuming for the time being its correctness, and knowing the well-established facts, (i) that gray pigmentation in rats is dominant over black (Crampe, Bateson, Doncaster) and (2) that every sort of pigmentation is dominant over albinism, we reach the following conclusion. On Mendelian principles we may expect partial albinos to fall into eighteen actually different classes, though visibly the classes number only four, namely, gray Irish, black Irish, gray hooded, and black hooded. These eighteen classes are enumerated in the four central columns of table i , page 34. Of gray Irish individuals, there should be eight actually different classes as regards gametic output; of black Irish and gray hooded indi- viduals, there should be four classes each; and of black hooded, two classes. Albinos, though indistinguishable in appearance, should fall into nine different classes as regards the latent pigment characters and color-patterns which they transmit. The nature of these nine classes is indicated in the last column of table i. The numerals prefixed to the class designations in table i indicate the frequencies in which the various classes may be expected to occur as a result of a particular sort of mating, namely, mating inter se gray Irish individuals which bear recessive the three characters, total albin- ism, black pigmentation, and hooded pattern, individuals designated GI(W.BH). The table is based on the assumption that gray Irish indi- viduals of the sort indicated are Mendelian triple heterozygotes, the three independent pairs of allelomorphic characters being pigmentation versus albinism, gray -versus black, and Irish versus hooded. In support of the assumption mentioned, it may be said that by proper breeding tests a majority of the hypothetical classes have been shown to exist. Thus, of the eighteen hypothetical pigmented classes enumerated in table i, all except four have been shown to occur, those four being GKW), GI(BH}* BI* and BI(W)*. Further, five out of the nine supposed classes of albinos have been proved *Demonstrated to exist since the foregoing was written. IN RATS AND GUINEA-PIGS. 9 to occur, those the existence of which has not yet been demonstrated being W[BI], W[GI], W[G-IH], and W[GB-I]. It is probable that more extensive tests would demonstrate the existence of all the missing classes, as no special search has been made for them, the demonstrations obtained being for the most part incidental to other investigations. The fundamental assumption on which table i rests finds further justifi- cation in the numerical proportions in which the various classes are observed to occur, as will appear from an examination of tables 2 and 3 (pp. 35, 36). In these tables are shown the theoretical Mendelian results and the actual results of twenty-three different sorts of matings involving the pigments and color-patterns which have been discussed in the previous pages. Table 2 includes only matings between pigmented animals; table 3, matings be- tween a pigmented animal and an albino. Throughout both tables, it will be seen, the expected Mendelian results agree quite closely with those actually observed. Two animals possessing the same recessive character have in no case produced offspring bearing the corresponding dominant character. Further, the proportions of dominants and of recessives observed in mixed lots of young agree in general quite closely with those expected. The most striking deviation is an excess of black hooded young (BH) which is seen in the totals for both table 2 and table 3. This, however, is not large enough and does not occur with enough uniformity to warrant one in regarding it as other than accidental. The totals for tables 2 and 3 combined are: Gl GH BI BH W Total. Observed 69 132 130 186 168 685 Expected 56 137.5 136 158 187 In order of size the groups are as expected, except the last two, in case of which the order is reversed. In addition to the experiments recorded in tables 2 and 3, the following observations may be mentioned as corroborative evidence of the Mendelian behavior of pigment characters, color-patterns, and albinism in rats. Black hooded individuals bearing white recessive, mated inter sc, give two classes of offspring, albinos and black hooded. These albinos have been shown to bear without exception the black hooded pattern in a latent con- dition. Of the black hooded offspring, there should be two classes — pure hooded (free from albinism) and hooded individuals bearing albinism re- cessive, the latter being twice as numerous as the former. Out of 22 off- spring tested, 14 had albinism recessive and 8 were pure hooded individuals. Matings of pure black hooded individuals with black hooded ones bearing white recessive gave all black hooded offspring, 109 in number. One-half of these should be pure hooded, and of the limited number tested this was found to be true. 10 INHERITANCE OF COAT-PIGMENTS AND COAT-PATTERNS The evidence contained in tables 2 and 3 (pp. 35, 36), is presented in the form of summaries only. To give an idea of the sources from which it was obtained, a genealogy may be considered in detail (fig. i). Wild gray . " o Wild white I £ IZ,W[BH] I 34, 16, GI(WBH) \ f 141 3 142 f 143 f 144 ). The data for inheritance of spots in the Dutch-marked series are con- tained in tables 5 to 14. The individuals are arranged in a series of groups, each group having a larger average amount of Dutch-marked ancestry than the foregoing one. For convenience in description, we may use the symbol D to designate any Dutch-marked animal, followed by a numeral to indicate the amount in generations of its Dutch-marked ancestry. On this basis an original Dutch-marked animal, one without Dutch-marked ancestors, would be designated D0; a Dutch-marked animal with one generation of Dutch- marked ancestry, D\ ; one with two generations of Dutch-marked ancestry, D2\ and one with three generations of Dutch-marked ancestry, D3. Simi- larly a Dutch-marked animal, one parent of which was Dutch-marked, but the other not, might be designated ZH ; the Dutch-marked young of D\ individuals might be called D\\ ; etc. The young of our Dutch-marked animals have been divided into four groups, the average amounts of Dutch-marked ancestry in these groups being respectively j^, i, 2, and 3 generations. Groups of individuals were at first created which came midway between the groups mentioned, but these differed so little from the adjacent groups that we decided finally to abolish them, dividing them equally between the groups to which they were intermediate. Thus a group, D\\, was divided equally between the groups DI and D2, and a group, D2\, between the groups D2 and D3. For each individual observed, a record was made of what spots it possessed and what spots its parents possessed. These records are shown in table8 5, 7, 9, and n for the four groups of individuals having the mean ancestral values £H, DI, D2, and D^ respectively. Each table contains separate records for head, shoulder, side, and rump spots. The significance of the various entries will be sufficiently clear after an examination of a single category (head spots) in table 5. There are for the individuals examined three pos- sible conditions as regards head spots. An individual may have two such spots (one on either side of the head) ; it may have only one head spot (right or left, as the case may be) ; or it may have no head spots. Records are made for these three conditions separately in the horizontal columns marked 2 spots, i spot, and o spots respectively. For the possible head- spot conditions of the two parents considered jointly, five categories are necessary, 4, 3, 2, i, and o. These are indicated in vertical columns in the tables. An entry in the upper left-hand square of the table signifies an individual having a pair of head spots, born of parents both of which were similarly marked; an entry in the next square to the right means an indi- vidual having likewise a pair of head spots, but born of parents only one of which had a pair of head spots, the other having one head spot only. The IN RATS AND GUINEA-PIGS. 21 table, accordingly, it will be observed, represents the summation of a large amount of detailed information concerning the distribution of spots in off- spring and parents, respectively. For convenience in comparing the categories of individuals one with another, the entries in the tables are expressed in per cents, as well as in numbers of cases observed. Thus in table 5, left-hand vertical column, entries are made concerning the head-spot conditions of 49 young born of parents both of which had two head spots. Of these 49 young, 42, or 85.7 per cent, had two head spots, as each of the parents had; 6, or 12.3 per cent, had only one head spot; while i individual, or 2 per cent, had no head spots. The other columns of the table are to be interpreted in a similar way. Glancing over the four divisions of table 5, which includes observations on i r 7 young, we notice that the offspring of parents both of which possess a particular pair of spots (left-hand vertical column) possess that same pair of spots in from 70 to 86 per cent of all cases. The agreement between parents and offspring is greatest (85.7 per cent) in the case of head spots, and least (70 per cent) in the case of side spots. From the last vertical column of table 5 we learn that when both parents lack shoulder or side spots, 50 to 58 per cent of their young lack the corre- sponding pair of spots entirely. These facts seem to indicate a rather strong degree of inheritance of par- ticular spots upon the body of the guinea-pig; so we at first interpreted them, but more extended observations limit this conclusion considerably. The four divisions of table 5 are combined in table 6, which accordingly shows, for the body as a whole, to what extent particular spots are inherited in the group of 117 individuals with which tables 5 and 6 deal. Inheritance of paired spots is seen to be, on the average, measured by 78.8 per cent (left- hand upper square, table 6) ; absence of paired spots is seen to be measured in inheritance by 55 percent (lower right-hand square, table 6). Further, the fewer spots the parents have, the fewer their young have, as a glance at the horizontal columns of table 6 will show. This is true not only of the body as a whole (table 6), but also of its individual parts (table 5). Par- ticular amounts of pigmentation are accordingly inherited quite strongly; distribution of that pigment in certain of the typically pigmented areas, rather than in others, is really not inherited, as we shall see, though such is the impression first given by these tables. For the second group of individuals of Dutch-marked ancestry (mean ancestral condition, DI), the observations concerning spot inheritance are given in tables 7 and 8; for the third group (D2), in tables 9 and 10; and for the fourth group (^3), in tables n and 12. The numbers of individuals in these several groups are 30, 178, and 136, respectively, making in the Dutch- marked series as a whole 461 individuals observed. 22 INHERITANCE OF COAT-PIGMENTS AND COAT-PATTERNS To obtain averages for the entire series, with which the values for the several groups might be compared, tables 5, 7, 9, and 1 1 have been combined to form table 13, which accordingly shows the average degree of inheritance, in the entire series, of head spots, shoulder spots, side spots, and rump spots, respectively. Similarly, tables 6, 8, io,and 12 have here been combined in table 14, which accordingly shows in general to what extent paired spots are inherited in the Dutch-marked series. A comparison of the inheritance coefficients (per cents) in corresponding places of tables 5, 7, 9, and n shows remarkably little change from one generation to another. Paired head spots are inherited in the four groups in 85.7, 82.1, 86. 5, and 89.3 per cent of the cases, respectively, or in the series as a whole, in 87 per cent of all cases (table 13, upper left-hand square). Similarly, paired rump spots are inherited in the four groups in 71.4, 80, 80.9, and 83.1 per cent of the cases, respectively, or in the series as a whole (table 13) in 80.7 per cent of all cases. In both head and rump spots the inheritance coefficients increase slightly with the progress of selection. Shoulder and side spots are so frequently lacking in the Dutch-marked series that comparison of the per cents for presence of these pairs would have little value; it is better to examine rather the per cents for absence of these spots. Both shoulder spots are absent in the four groups in 58.5, 59, 41, and 42 per cent of the cases, respectively (a decreasing series) ; average for the four groups, 46.3 per cent. Similarly, side spots are entirely wanting in the several groups in 50, 34.7, 47.2, and 44.8 per cent, respectively (again, on the whole, a decrease), the average for the four groups being 46 per cent. It is scarcely necessary to examine in detail other parts of the several tables. They show conditions for the most part intermediate between those seen in these extreme instances. Selection for presence of spots (head and rump) and for absence of spots (shoulder) seems, in the same series of observations, to give contrary results. These seemingly inconsistent facts are really not inconsistent. Spots desired present and selected for have become slightly oftener present; spots desired absent and selected against have likewise become slightly oftener present. Selection for particular spots has had nothing to do with bringing about the result observed. The pigmentation has become somewhat more extensive in the race, without our having consciously aimed to make it so. Spots occur somewhat more numerously in the later than in the earlier gen- erations of the series (see table 26). The increase, however, takes place no oftener in regions where increase is desired (head and rump) than where it is not desired (shoulder), or where its occurrence is a matter of indifference (side). This result shows the entire inefficiency of selection in guinea-pigs to fix a coat-pattern which is dependent upon the distribution of pigment in par- ticular spots upon the body. IN RATS AND GUINEA-PIGS. 23 Head spots regularly give higher inheritance coefficients than any other set of spots, a possible reason being that head spots really include two distinct pairs of pigment areas, namely, eye spots and ear spots. Commonly, in the Dutch-marked series, both of these are pigmented, in which case it makes no difference whether the two are dealt with separately or jointly But sometimes an eye area may be pigmented, while the adjacent ear area is unpigrnented, or vice versa; in such cases separate enumerations of eye and of ear spots would indicate a less amount of head pigmentation than the joint reckoning followed, for purposes of convenience, in this paper. Aside from this consideration, however, it is certain that the head region is oftener pigmented than any other part of the body. HEAD-SPOT SERIES (SERIES H). The data concerning spot inheritance in the head-spot series are contained in tables 15 to 24. The entries are made separately for the different regions of the body in tables 15, 17, 19, and 21, for four different groups of indi- viduals possessing different average amounts of head-spot ancestry. The average amounts of head-spot ancestry in these several groups are a half- generation, one generation, two generations, and three generations, respec- tively, and may be expressed by the symbols //£, HI, H2, and H3,a.s explained in connection with the Dutch-marked series. In the group H§ (table 15), one of the parents of each individual recorded bore certain spots other than head spots, but in the other three groups of Series// (tables 17, 19, and 21), none of the parents bore spots other than head spots. Tables 15, 17, 19, and 21 are combined in table 23, which shows the average degree of in- heritance of particular spots in the series as a whole. As in the case of Series D, the entries for the different regions of the body have, in the case of each group of individuals, been combined to get an expres- sion for spot inheritance in general, in the particular group of individuals under consideration. These combination entries are found in tables 16, 18, 20, and 22, for the four groups considered separately, and combined into one table for the whole head-spot series, in table 24. In this series, rigid selection has been exercised for head spots, and against all other spots. We will now consider what the effects of this two-fold selection are. Paired head spots are present in the following per cents of the young of parents possessing such spots, in the several groups: 79.8, 85.4, 77-i, and 82.1 per cent; average for the series (table 23),8i.i per cent.* No change is observed uniformly in the direction either of increase or of *In obtaining the average, each table is weighted in proportion to the number of individ- uals recorded in it. These are, for the successive groups, 207, 193, 129, and 58; total, 587. 24 INHERITANCE OF COAT-PIGMENTS AND COAT-PATTERNS decrease of head spots, but they are on the whole less common in this series than in Series D (81.1 and 87 per cent, respectively). However, shoulder, side, and rump spots become steadily less common in the series, as a glance at table 25 will show. It is doubtful, however, whether this is to be regarded as an effect of selection for elimination of specific areas. It indicates, rather, a decrease in the average amount of pigmentation pos- sessed by animals of this series, unwittingly brought about by the attempt to eliminate most of the spots typically pigmented (compare table 27). It is significant that head spots, though desired present and chosen equally in Series D and in Series H, occur more commonly in the former than in the latter. Animals of Series D are more extensively pigmented than those of Series H (compare tables 26 and 27) ; the additional pigmentation may occur in any of the typically pigmented areas, regardless of the areas which happen to have been pigmented in the selected ancestors. Nevertheless, we find a more rapid falling off in the pigmentation of some areas than in that of others. In Series H, shoulder spots fall off 16.9 per cent; side spots, 7.3 per cent; and rump spots, 6.4 per cent; while head spots, as already noted, show little change. Now, selection, it will be remem- bered, was exercised for elimination of shoulder, side, and rump spots alike, but for the retention of head spots. Accordingly we do not get changes uniformly consistent with selection. Regardless of selection for specific spots, we get, when the total amount of the pigmentation is decreasing, a decrease most rapidly in shoulder spots, less rapidly in side spots, less rapidly still in rump spots, while the headispots are least affected. Conversely, in a series in which the amount of pigmentation is increasing, the increase is less marked in head than in rump spots, and less in side than in shoulder spots. Thus, in Series D, head spots increase 3.6 per cent, rump spots, 11.7 per cent; the change in side spots (entire absence) is 6.2 per cent, of shoulder spots, 16.5 per cent. In further support of this same idea — that with increasing or decreasing pigmentation changes occur more rapidly in some areas than in others, irrespective of the particular spots borne by the ancestors — we may com- pare the averages for Series D and H (tables 13 and 23). The differences between the two series are, in frequencies of head spots, 5.9 per cent; of side spots (absent), 20 per cent; of shoulder spots (absent), 25.4 per cent. As regards rump spots, no comparable entries occur in either the first or the last columns of tables 13 and 23, but an examination of the middle column (2) in both tables indicates that the difference in rump spots in the two series is about as great as in head spots. These facts are in harmony with an observation made in advance of the statistical investigation, that the most persistent of all the pigmented areas are the head spots, and next in order of persistency are the rump spots. IN RATS AND GUINEA-PIGS. 25 Our general conclusion from a study of these two series of guinea-pigs, includ- ing 1,048 individuals, is that one can by selection either increase or decrease the extent of the pigmented areas, but it is impossible by selection to fix this pigmentation in a particular pattern, retaining pigment areas on certain parts of the body and eliminating them from others. As the pigmentation changes in extent, under the influence of selection, the -various areas typically pigmented are affected in the following order: Shoulder, side, rump, and head, the change being greatest in the first-named and least in the last-named area, irrespective of what particular spots were present in the selected ancestors. STATISTICAL ANALYSIS OF THE DATA FOR SERIES D AND H. The foregoing conclusions may, we think, fairly be drawn from an exami- nation of the tables as they stand. For those, however, who place confidence in the more precise methods of statistical analysis devised by Pearson and others, it may be more satisfactory to treat the tables, which have been constructed for the various groups of individuals, as correlation tables, and derive from them the constants which measure the variability of parents and children respectively in the several groups, and the degree of correlation between the two. Such constants are given, in tables 26 and 27, for the several groups of Series D and H, respectively. From the left-hand columns of table 26 we learn that, in Series D, the number of spots borne by an individual increases from group to group, in the case of both parents and offspring; but the number of spots is, in every case, 20 to 26 per cent greater in the offspring than in their parents. This indicates a tendency for the offspring to become pigmented in regions which lacked pigment in their selected parents. Table 27 shows the existence of a similar, but still stronger, tendency in Series H, the offspring bearing 50 to 105 per cent more spots than their parents. This tendency may be considered regression, a tendency to return to a condition of more widely distributed pigmentation, and to acquire spots where the selected ancestors lacked them. It does not imply that the young bear more pigment than their parents. The regression is stronger the more reduced the pigmentation of the parents, as we might expect. This is seen to be true both within Series H and in that series as a whole compared with Series D. The standard deviation from the average number of spots is for the offspring a very constant quantity, being close to 3.6 in both Series D and Series H. This indicates no change in the variability in number of spots as a result of selection, or as a consequence of change in the number of pig- mented areas. The parents show throughout Series H and in Group D\ of Series D a less standard deviation than their offspring. This fact, however, 26 INHERITANCE OF COAT-PIGMENTS AND COAT-FATTERNS has for our purposes little significance, since the parents are selected indi- viduals, while their offspring are not. The same may be said of the coeffi- cients of variability for the parents. For the offspring, the coefficient of variability (ratio of standard deviation to mean, times TOO) decreases slightly in Series D, and increases in Series H. This must not be interpreted as signifying a change in variability of an opposite nature in the two series. It is due entirely to the changes in the mean (average number of spots), these changes being opposite in nature in the two series. If the mean re- mained constant throughout each series, the amount of variability indicated by the coefficient of variability would likewise remain constant ; for we have seen that the standard deviation is constant, irrespective of changes in the mean and irrespective of the number of generations during which selection has been in progress. Standard deviation is, therefore, a better measure of variability than the coefficient of variability in these series. The coefficient of correlation (r) has little real significance in tables 26 and 27. This constant is relatively small in the Dutch-marked series and grows smaller as selection progresses, whereas in the head-spot series it is relatively large and grows larger with the progress of selection. Selection does not have an opposite effect in the two series upon the inheritance of a coat-pattern. The whole effect is due to change in the amount of the pigmentation, not to its distribution. When the amount of pigmented surface is small, its distribution in certain spots (head and rump) is more certain and the correlation rises (Series H). When, on the other hand, the pigmented surface is large (Series D, table 26), its distribution is less certain and the correlation is low; it becomes lower as the pigmentation increases, in spite of progressive selection. The statistical analysis confirms the conclusions previously drawn from our observations. Selection is powerless to fix a particular coat-pattern not dependent upon amount of pigmentation. It is as powerless to decrease the variability in number of spots as to fix any pattern formed by them. NOSE-SPOT SERIES (SERIES N). It is only in the case of the nose-spot series (Series N) that we are able to detect influence of selection in fixing a pigment-pattern among guinea-pigs. Two individuals showing this rather striking variation in pattern were figured by Castle (105, pi. 6); another 'is shown in pi. 2, fig. i, of this paper. The starting-point of the series of selections for the fixation of this pattern was the male 1989° shown in fig. 12, pi. 6 (Castle, 105). Besides the nose spot shown in the figure, this animal bore a distinct right shoulder spot, not visible in the figure. This animal produced a considerable number of young with nose-spot markings similar to his own, as will be seen from an examina- tion of table 28. As a result of matings with animals which did not bear IN RATS AND GUINEA-PIGS. 2j nose spots, he produced four individuals with nose spots and fourteen with- out nose spots, or 22.2 per cent of nose-spot (NS) young. When he was mated with these nose-spot young (N S%) or with nose-spot individuals produced in other experiments but not of nose-spot ancestry, and so desig- nated NSo, he produced a much higher percentage of nose-spot young, viz, 53.6 per cent by NSo mothers, and 61.5 per cent by N S% mothers. In one case he was mated with his NS\ daughter by an N SQ mother, from which mating there resulted three nose-spot young and two otherwise marked, or 60 per cent NS. The number of young in this last experiment is too small to be very significant quantitatively, but those in the other matings are large enough. They show an increase in the per cent of NS young with an increase in the amount of NS ancestry, and strongly suggested the possi- bility of a still further increase by continued selection. Such increase, however, has not up to this time been realized. Five sons or grandsons of cT 1989° have been quite extensively tested and the same is true of one individual ( ... 25 6 4 7'-4 17.2 11.4 ... ... 4i 20 21 5« 24-4 25.6 ... ... ... • 35 ... ... ... 82 •• TABLE 6. — Combination of the four parts of table 5, showing in general the extent to which pigment spots are inherited in group D%, 7/7 individuals (468 observations). Parents. Total Offspring. 4 spots. 3 spots. 2 SpOtS. I spot. o spots. of cases. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. Cases. ;P. ct. Cases. P. ct. 2 SDOtS ... 78 78.8 16 55-2 IOO 50-5 3 ... 27 19.6 I I spot ... 13 13 2 1 1 37-9 49 24-7 i * • • 35 254 1 o spots... 8 80 2 6.9 49 247 ... ... 76 55-o [468 Total... 99 ... 29 ... 198 ... 4 138 ... 38 INHERITANCE OF COAT-PIGMENTS AND COAT-PATTERNS TABLE 7. — -Relation between distribution oj pigment spots in }O guinea-pigs of group D\, and in their parents. Off-.pring. Parents. Location of spots and total number of cases. 4 spots. 3 spots. 2 Spots. i spot. o spots. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. 2 Spots. i &pot. . o s-pots. Total.. 2 Spots. i spot., o spots. Total.. 2 SpOtS. i spot. . o spots. Total.. 2 Spots. j spot. . o spots. Total.. 23 5 82.1 17.9 I I ... ... ... | Head; J- total cases, 30- | Shoulder; } total cases, 130 | Side; } total cases, 130- 1 | Rump; [. total cases, 130- J ... ... ... ... 28 ... 2 2 ••> I ... ... ... ... ... ... ... ... I I I 4 5 13 18.3 22.7 59-o ... ... 5 ... ... 3 ... 22 ... ... ... ... ... 2 2 ... ... ... 13 4 9 500 15-3 347 ... ... ... ... 4 ... ... ... 26 ... 24 3 3 So IO IO ... ... ... ... 30 ... ... ... ... ... TABLE 8. — Combination of the four parts of table 7, showing spot inheritance in group D\, }o individuals (/ 20 observations'). Parents. Total number Offspring. 4 spots. 3 spots. 2 SpOtS. i spot. o spots. of cases. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. 2 Spots... I spot.... 47 8 Si.o 13-8 3 3 ... 2 2 ... I I ... 17 9 354 18.8 1 o spots... 3 5-2 i ... ... ... I ... 22 45-3 J- 120 Total.... 58 ... 7 ... 4 • • 3 ... 48 ... J IN RATS AND GUINEA-PIGS. 39 TABLE 9. — Relation between distribution of pigment spots in 178 guinea-pigs of group D2, and in their parents. Offspring. Parents. Location of spots and total number of cases. 4 spots. 3 spots. 2 Spots. i spot. o spots. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. 2 Spots. i spot., o spots. Total. 2 SpOtS. i spot., o spots. Total . 2 SpOttS. i spot., o spots. Total. 2 Spots. i spot. . o spots. Total. 154 24 86.5 13-5 ... ... ... ... | Head ; }• total cases, 1 173. 1 | Shoulder; }• total cases, 1 178. 1 Side; > total cases, 1 178. 1 Rump ; y total cases, 1 178- 178 ... ... ... ... ... ... ... 3 i i ... 5 7 7 26.3 36.8 368 7 4 4 46.7 26.7 26.7 49 33 57 353 23-7 41. ... ... 5 ... !9 ... 15 ... 139 ... 6 ••» 3 54-5 18.2 27-3 4 2 ... 22 7 12 53-6 17.1 29-3 5 i 6 41.7 8-3 50. 39 18 5i 36.1 16.7 47.2 ii ... 6 ... 41 ... 12 ... 108 ... 144 16 18 80.9 9.0 TO. I ... ... ... ... 178 ... ... ... ... ... TABLE 10. — Combination o] the four parts of table 9, showing spot inheritance in group Z?2, 178 individuals (712 observations). Parents. Total Offspring. 4 spots. 3 spots. 2 Spots. i spot. o spots. of cases. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. 2 Spots... 304 82.8 7 6.3.6 27 45-o 12 44-4 88 35.6 1 i spot. .. 42 II.4 3 27-3 H 23 3 5 18.5 5i 20.7 | o spots... 21 5-7 i 9-1 19 3i-7 10 37-o 1 08 43-7 j-712 Total... 367 ... ii ... 60 ... 37 ... 247 ... J INHERITANCE OF COAT-PIGMENTS AND COAT-PATTERNS TABLE n. — Relation between distribution of pigment spots in i}6 guinea-pigs of group D$, and in their parents. Offspring. Parents. Location of spots and total number of cases. 4 spots. 3 spots. 2 SpOtS. i spot. o spots. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. 2 spots. i spot., o spots. Total. 2 Spots. i spot. . o spots. Total . 2 SpOtS. i spot. . o spots. Total. 2 Spots. I spot., o spots. Total . 117 H 893 10.7 4 I 80 2O ... ... | Head ; } total cases, 1 136. 1 | Shoulder; } total cases, 1 136- | Side; J total cases, 136- 1 | Rump ; } total cases. h36. ... ... ... ... 'SI ... 5 • • • ... ... ... 2 I ... ... ... I I ... 48 28 55 36.6 21.4 42.0 ... ... 3 ... ... ... 2 ... i.3' ... 2 2 2 33- 33- 33- ... ... 3S H 20 52.8 19 4 27.8 ... ... '9 i3 26 32-8 224 44.8 6 • • • • • • ... 72 ... ... ... 58 ... "3 H 9 83.1 10.3 6.6 ... ... ... ... 136 ... ... ... ... ... TABLE 12. — Combination of the four parts of table 1 1, showing spot inheritance in group D3, i}6 individuals (544 observations). Parents. Total number Offspring. 4 spots. 3 spots. ? spots. i spot. o spots. of Cases. P. rt. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. 2 Spots... 232 85.0 6 75 38 52-8 I ... 67 35-5 1 i spot 3° I I.O 2 25 J4 19.4 I ... 4' 21.7 1 o spots... n 4.0 ... 20 27.8 ... ... 81 42.8 ^544 | Total.... 273 ... 8 ... 7^ ... 2 ... 189 ... J IN RATS AND GUINEA-PIGS. TABLE 13. — Combinations separately of tlie four parts of tables 5, 7, 9, and 11, showing inheritance of particular spots in series D as a whole, 461 individuals. Off spins. Parents. Location of spots and total number of cases. 4 spots. 3 spots. 2 SpOtS. i spot. o spots. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. 2 Spots. i spot., o spots. Total . 2 Spots. i spot., o spots. Total.. 2 Spots i spot., o spots. Total.. 2 Spots. i spot. . o spots. Total . 336 49 i 87.0 12.7 °3 15 10 60 40 35 i5 70 30 I Head; }• total cases, 461- 1 | Shoulder; J- total cases, |46i. | Side ; } total ca^es, l46i. 1 | Rump ; J- total cases, (461. J 3§6 ... 25 ... .So ... ... ... ... ... 4 i 80 20 10 7 4 47.6 33-3 HJ.O 13 ii 17 31-7 26.8 41-5 9 5 6 45 o 25.0 30.0 114 87 173 305 23.2 46.3 5 ... 21 ... 4i ... 20 ... 374 ... 15 4 8 55-6 14.8 29.6 7 2 77.8 22.2 78 33 5° 48.4 20.5 4I.O 8 2 6 50.0 12.5 37-5 85 49 114 34-3 19.7 46.0 27 ... 9 ... 161 ... 16 • • • 248 ... 306 39 34 80.7 10.3 9.0 ... ... 4i 20 21 500 24.4 25.6 379 ... ... ... 82 ... 1 TABLE 14. — Combination oj tables 6, 8, 10, and /?, showing spot inheritance in series D in general, 461 individuals (1,844 observations). Offspring. Parents. Total number of cases. 4 spots. 3 spots. 2 Spots. i spot. o spots. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. 2 Spots.. i spot — o spots... Total ... 66 1 93 43 82.9 11.7 5-4 32 19 4 58.2 345 7-3 I67 79 88 50.0 23-7 26-3 17 8 1 1 47-2 22 2 30.6 199 136 287 32.0 21.9 46.1 M§44 797 ... 55 ... 334 ... 36 ... 622 ... INHERITANCE OF COAT-PIGMENTS AND COAT-PATTERNS TABLE 15. — Relation between distribution of pigment spots in 207 guinea-pigs of group H%, and in their parents. Offspring. Parents. Location of spots and total number of cases. 4 spots. 3 spots. 2 spots. i spot. o spots. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. Cases P. ct. 2 spots. i spot.. o spots. Total. 2 spots. i spot. . o spots. Total . 2 Spots. I spot.. o spots. Total. 2 SpOtS . i spot.. o spots. Total. 8.3 12 9 79.8 "•5 87 20 13 60.6 394 35 15 2 673 28.8 3-8 IO 3 76.9 23 1 3 2 60. 40. | Head ; }• total cases, 1 207. 1 | Shoulder; J- total cases, 1 207. | Side; J- total cases, 1 207. 1 | Rump; j- total cases, 207. 104 ... 33 ... 52 ... i3 ... 5 ... ... ... = ... 14 7 21 33-3 16 7 50.0 ... ... 27 32 1 06 16.4 194 64.2 ... ... ... ... 42 ... ... ... '65 ... ... ... ... ... 20 9 21 40.0 18.0 42 o i 5 5 9.1 45-4 45-4 25 31 90 17.1 21.2 61.7 ... ... ... ... 50 ... ii ... 146 ... ... ... ... ... 44 17 23 524 20 2 27.4 IO 8 ii 34-5 27.6 379 26 M 54 27.7 14.9 57-4 ... ... ... ... 84 ... 29 ... 94 ... TABLE 16. — Combination of the four parts of table 15, showing spot inheritance in group H$, 207 individuals (828 observations). Offspring. Parents. Total number of cases. 4 spots. 3 spots. 2 SpOtS. i spot. o spots. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. 2 Spots... i spot.... o spots... Total.... 83 12 9 79.8 "•5 8.7 20 13 60.6 39-4 "3 48 67 49.6 21.0 29.4 21 16 16 396 30.2 30.2 Si 77 252 19.7 iS.S 61.5 1 !>82S j 104 ... 33 ... 228 ... 53 ... 410 ... IN RATS AND GUINEA-PIGS. TABLE 17. — Relation between distribution 0} pigment spots in group Hi, and in their parents. guinea-pigs o) Offspring. Parents. Location of spots and total number of cases. 4 spots. 3 spots. 2 SpOtS. i spot. o spots. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. Jnses. P. Ct. 2 spots. i spot., o spots. Total . 2 Spots. I spot., o spots. Total . 2 Spots. i spot. . o spots. Total. 2 Spots. i spot. . o spots. Total. 88 ii 4 854 10.7 3-9 13 5 i 68.4 26.3 5-3 37 21 7 570 32-3 107 3 i 2 500 16.7 333 : ... | Head ; |- total cases, 1 193- j Shoulder; ^ total cases, 1 193- 1 Side; > total cases, 1 193- 1 | Rump; V total cases, i '93- 103 ... !9 ... 65 ... 6 ... ... ... ... ... ... ... • * * ... H 140 39 7-3 20.7 72.0 •• ... ... • • * ... • • ... ... 193 ... • • • ... ... • « ... ... • • • 29 35 129 15.0 iS.i 66.8 •• ... ... ... ... ... ... 193 ... ... ... ... ... ... ... ... ... 54 108 28.0 16.0 56.0 ... ... ... ... ... ... ... ... 193 | TABLE 1 8. — Combination of the jour parts of table //, showing spot inheritance in group H\, 193 individuals (772 observations). Offspring. Parents. Total number of cases. 4 spots. 3 spots. 2 SpOtS. i spot. o spots. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. 2 Spots.. i spot.... o spots... Total.... 88 1 1 4 85-4 10.7 3-9 13 5 i 684 263 5-3 37 21 7 56-9 323 108 3 i 2 50 167 33-3 97 1 06 376 16.8 18.3 64.9 } !>772 J 103 !9 ... 65 ... 6 ... 579 • • 44 INHERITANCE OF COAT-PIGMENTS AND COAT-PATTERNS TABLE 19. — Relation between distribution of pigment spots in 129 guinea-pigs of group //2, and in their parents. Offspring. Parents. Locaiion of spots and total number of cases. 4 spots. 3 spots. 2 SpOtS. I J-pOt. o spots. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. Ct. Ca--es. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. 2 spots. i spot. . o spots. Total.. 2 spots. I spot. . o spots. Total. . 2 Spots. i spot. . o spots. Total.. 2 Spots. i spot., o spots. Total.. 74 20 2 77 j 20.8 2.1 10 2 83-3 16.7 17 4 81.0 19 o | Head ; } total cases, J139' I Shoulder ; \ total cases, |-I29. | Side; } total cases, 1 129. 1 I Rump; } total cases, 1 129. 96 ... 12 ... 21 ... ... ... • • • ... ... ... ... ... 4 26 99 3-i 20 2 76.7 ... ... ... ... ... ... ... ... 129 ... ... ... • * • • • ... — - — ... ... ii 3° 88 8-5 23-3 68.2 ... ... ... ... ... ... 129 ... ... ... * • • ... ... ... ... 36 17 76 27.9 !3-2 589 ... ... ... ... ... ... 129 ... TABLE 20. — Combination of the jour parts of table 19, showing spot inheritance in group //2, 129 individuals (5 / 6 observations) . Offspring. Parents. Total number of cases. 4 spots. 3 spots. 2 SpOtS. j spot. o spots. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. Ct. Case?. P. ct. P. ct. P. ct. 2 Spots... i spot — o spots... Total.. . 74 20 2 77 i 20.8 2.1 10 2 83-3 16.7 17 4 81 o 19.0 ... E 51 73 263 13.2 18.8 68.0 |SI6 96 ... 12 ... 21 ... ... 387 ... IN RATS AND GUINEA-PIGS. TABLE 21. — Relation between distribution of pigment spots in 58 guinea-pigs o] group //3, and in their parents. 45 Offspring. Parents. Location of spots and total number of cases. 4 spots. 3 spots. 2 spots. i spot. o spots. Cases. P. ct. C°ses. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. 2 spots. . I spot . . o spots. . Total.. 2 Spots. . i spot . . o spots.. Total . . 2 Spots. . i spot . . o spots.. Total.. 2 Spots . i spot. . o spots . Total . . 46 IO 82.1 17.9 I I E ... I Head ; J total cases, 5S. Shoulder; ;> total cases, jsl. | Side; \ total cases, | Rump ; \ total cases, |5. ... 56 ... 2 ... ... ... ... ... ... ... ... ... ... ... 2 9 47 34 '5-5 Si. i ... ... ... ... ... ... ... 58 ... E ... ... ... ... ... ... ... 4 14 40 6.9 24.1 69.0 ... ... ... ... • • . ... ... ... 58 ... ... ... ... ... ... ... ... 13 8 37 22.4 13.8 63.8 ... ... ... ... ... ... ... 58 ... TABLE 22. — Combination of the jour parts of table 21 , showing spot inheritance in group Hz, 58 individuals (232 observations). Parents. Total Offspring. 4 spots. 3 spots. 2 SpOtS. i spot. f spots. number of Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. cases. 2 Spots... 46 82.1 I ... ... ... ... ... 19 10.9 1 i spot.... 10 17.9 1 ... ... ... ... ... 31 17.8 o spots... ... ... ... ... ... ... ... ... 124 71-3 [232 Total... 56 ... 2 .,. ... ... ... ... 174 ... J 46 INHERITANCE OF COAT-PIGMENTS AND COAT-PATTERNS TABLE 23.— Combinations separately of the jour parts of tables 75, /;, 19, and 21, showing inheritance of particular spots in series H as a whole, $87 individuals. Parents. Location of Offspring. 4 spots. 3 spots. 2 SpOtS. i spot. o spots. spots and total number of Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. 2 spots. I spot. . o spots. 291 53 15 Si. i 14.7 4-2 44 21 I 66.7 31.8 89 40 9 64-5 29.0 6-5 13 4 2 68.4 21. 1 10.5 3 2 60. 40. Head; > total cases, "587. Total. 359 ... 66 ... 138 ... 19 ... 5 ... 2 Spots. i spot. . o spots. ... ... ... ... H 7 21 33-3 16.7 •• ... 47 107 18.6 19.7 71.7 | Shoulder; y total cases, 1587. Total. ... ... ... ... 42 ... ... ... 545 ... 2 Spots. ... 20 40. I 9.1 69 13-1 I spot., o spots. • • ... ... ... 9 21 18. 42. 5 5 45-4 45-4 no 347 20.9 66.0 1 Side; y total cases, Total. ... ... ... ... 50 ... ii ... 526 ... J 2 Spots. 44 52-4 10 34-5 129 27.2 I spot. . o spots. ... ... ... ... 17 23 20. 2 27.4 8 ii 27.6 37-9 70 275 14.8 58.0 Rump ; total cases, 1587. 5 7 Total ... ... ... ... 1 ... 39 • • * 474 ... TABLE 24. — Combination 0} tables 16, 18, 20, and 22, showing spot inheritance in series H in general, 587 individuals (2348 observations). Offspring. Parents. Total number of cases. 4 spots. 3 spots. 2 Spots. i spot. o spots. Cases. 29I 53 15 P. ct. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. Cases. P. ct. 40.7 28.8 30-5 Cases. P. ct. 2 spots... i spot.... o spots... Total... 81.1 14.7 4.2 44 21 66.7 31.8 i-5 I67 73 74 53-2 23-3 23-5 24 17 18 248 287 1015 16.0 18.5 65-5 j>2348 359 66 ... 3H ... 59 ... 1550 ... TABLE 25. — Frequencies -with -which spots absent in the parents (or in one parent, in group //i) are absent also in their offspring. Group HJ (table 15). Group Hi (table 17). Group Ha (table 19). Group H$ (table 21). Average (table 33). Shoulder spots Per ct. 64.2 61.7 Per ct. 72 o 66.8 Per et. 76.7 68.2 Perct. Si I 69 o Per ct. 71.7 66.0 RuiTip spots S7-4 <;6 o 58-9 638 58.0 IN RATS AND GUINEA-PIGS. 47 TABLE 26 — Constants for the four groups of series D, based upon tables 6, 8, 10, and 12, respectively. Group DJ. Group Di. Group Dz. Group 03. Average number of spots borne by a parent •? 78 4.4O 4 62 4.64 Average number of spots borne by offspring... 4.76 c.44 5-$6 c 72 Per cent increase over parents .... Standard deviation (a} in num- ber of spots, parents 26 2.876 24 T.Sl2 20 •1.744. 23 -i 604 Standard deviation (a) in num- ber of spots, offspring -j CQ'? •j.6ic 1.661 T. 662 Coefficient of variability, par- ents j j^j 76 78 J-^'O 8s "?2 J-v^'O So DO 7Q AA Coefficient of variability, off- surinsr. . |>j. yu 72 Q2 UO" J* 67.70 6c 70 /y-TT 64 20 Coefficient of correlation be- tween parents and offspring.. / *-y* 0.4^27 "-1/' /y 0.4580 0.4278 0-4343 TABLE 27. — Constants for the jour groups of individuals of series H, based upon tables 1 6, 18, 20, and 22, respectively. Group HJ. Group Hi. Group 11?. Group H3- Average number of spots borne by a parent 2 48 I l6 I 80 2 OO Average number of spots borne by offspring -.^u 7 88 i .^u 1 2O •? 08 •3 on Per cent increase over parents .... Standard deviation (,r) in num- ber of spots, parents 56 2 870 S-^^j 105 2.022 ).UU 71 T.IQS _J.UU 5° •> A 14 Standard deviation (^) in num- ber of spots, offspring T. <;'768 -\ 6076 3co6o o-4j4 o ^i^no Coefficient of variability, par- ents OO/UJ I 1C. 72 _}. t. £ — u? 0 >- ^ c ^ •/ •u-^ o. o 2 n M °- •a o a. o J2 ^^ P1 c ^ o. • — & V o "rt 0- > 3 ">S u — o a o. u c i_ 4J tJi ry: 0 C U c o 4-t .c c?5595' NS0 by NS0 mothers I 5 5- 6 27 45 H-3 " " NSt " 4 10 5 15 3- 44-4 (^5678, NS, " NSo " O o 0. 2 5 2-5 o. " " NS, " 2 6 3- 3 8 2-7 40. tfS*5i, NS2 " NS0 " 15 30 2. 22 66 3- 4°-5 " " NSj " 9 32 3-5 13 52 4- 409 ^5652, NS2, " NSo 7 16 2-3 5 J3 2.6 5S.3 I 4 ti U N^ ^ I 4 2 3 1.5 2 9 4-5 50- CT53SS, NS,J, " NSo 0 o o. 4 3-5 o. " , " NSi 5 6 1.2 6 ii 1.8 45-4 C? "5669, NS2*, " NSo i 3 3- 4 10 2-5 20. " , " NSi " 2 5 2-5 i i i. 67. " , " NS2 I 5 5- i 4 4- 50. Total 121 2.C 74 27C O O 39.8 O / T jO ' O.7 TABLE 30. — Further analysis of the data contained in table 29. (/: - V ' w. ^ K O ^" w •3_d o •£ «; ^^ S. -u § §• *» i« W)^ ^ ^i -r o c ? o. rs.- S* ^ o i! s i "> "£ * o S E H' S 60 " > p u -5 "i o c # o o <; c,c Sj § o M C H o ™ c H o ^^ NS0 males bv NS0 females i 5 5-° 8 33 40 1 1 NSo males by NSi females 6 16 2-7 8 23 2.9 43 NSi or i £ males by NSo females. 23 2.1 35 103 2.9 396 NSi or jjmalesbyNSi females. 16 43 2-7 20 64 3-2 44 NSi or ii males by NSj females. 3 S 2.7 3 13 4-3 5° Totals for NSo males 7 21 3.0 1 6 5S 34 3°4 Totals for NSt or Tj males... IOO 2.2 58 1 80 42 Totals for NS0 females 24 S4 2.1 43 ns 3-1 35-8 Totals for NSi females 2 7 28 87 3-1 44 Totals for NS2 females T. 8 2 7 3 13 43 Grand totals 4Q 121 2.5 74 23S 3-2 398 IN RATS AND GUINEA-PIGS- 49 TABLE 31. — Character as regards nose spots o) the young of animals of Series H (head spot series). None of the parents bore nose spots. U- en "rt x U rt *- •- jj Wl ^ u n O "5 •g 3 o V C u > 3 U < c-£ •--s e*l O > 3 4-5 7-/-J ... y -jj. 4-4 2 I IO i i i c 76 K 16 Is 16 9 Q i i 22 2 L A IO q 1 1 •j 76.2 M i 2 4 * 6 c i O" ri 4 J"'T BIBLIOGRAPHY. ALLEN, G. M. : 04. The heredity of coat-color in mice. Proc. Amer. Acad. Arts and Sci., vol. 40, No. 2, pp. 61-163, 2 fig3- BATESON, W. '94. Materials for the study of variation, treated with especial regard to discon- tinuity in the origin of species, xv+598 pp. Macmillan & Co., London and New York. BATESON, W. : 03. The present state of knowledge of colour-heredity in mice and rats. Proc. Zool. Soc. London, 1903, vol. 2, pp. 71-93. CASTLE, W. E. : 05. Heredity of coat-characters in guinea-pigs and rabbits. Carnegie Institution Publication No. 23, 78 pp., 6 pi. CASTLE, W. E. : 06. The origin of a polydactylous race of guinea-pigs. Carnegie Institution Pub- lication No. 49, pp. 15-29. CASTLE, W. E., and ALLEN, G. M. :o3. The heredity of albinism. Proc. Amer. Acad. Arts and Sci., vol. 38, No. 21, pp. 603-622. CASTLE, W. E., and FORBES, A. : 06. Heredity of hair-length in guinea-pigs and its bearing on the theory of pure gametes. Carnegie Institution Publication No. 49, pp. 3-14. CORRENS, C. :oi. Die Ergebnisse der neusten Bastardforschungen fur die Vererbungslehre. Bericht der Deutsch. Botanischen Gesellschaft, Bd. 19, pp. 71-94. CRAMPE. '77-84. Kreuzungen zwischen Wanderratten verschiedener Farbe. Landwirths. Jahrb., Bd. 6, p. 384; Bd. 12, p. 389; Bd. 13, p. 692. '85. Die Gesetze der Vererbung der Farbe. Landwirths. Jahrb., Bd. 14, p. 539. CUENOT, L. : 02. La loi de Mendel et I'he'r&lite' de la pigmentation chez les souris. Arch. Zool. Exp£r. et G£n., Se>. 3, Tom. 10, Notes et Revue, pp. xxvii-xxx. DARBISHIRE, A. D. : 03. Second report on the result of crossing Japanese waltzing mice with European albino races. Biometrika, vol. 2, pp. 165-173, figs. 1-6. DONCASTER, L. : 06. On the inheritance of coat-colour in rats. Proc. Cambridge Phil. Soc., vol. 13, pp. 215-227. HURST, C. C. : 05. Experimental studies on heredity in rabbits. Jour. Linnean Society, vol. 29, PP- 283-324. LOEB, LEO. '97. Ueber Transplantation von weisser Haut auf einem Defect in schwarzer Haut und umgekehrt am Ohr des Meerschweinchens. Arch. Entw.-Mech., Bd. 6, PP- i-44, 3 Taf., 2 Fig. MULLER, FRITZ. '86. Ein Ziichtungsversuch an Mais. Kosmos, Bd. 19, pp. 22-26 VRIES, H. DE. : 01-03. Die Mutationstheorie. Versuche und Beobachtungen iiber die Entstehung der Arten im Pflanzenreich. Bd. i, xii + 648 pp., 8 Taf.; Bd. 2, xiv-f 752 pp., 4 Taf. Ye it & Co., Leipzig. : 03. Species and varieties; their origin by mutation, xviii+847 pp. The Open Court Pub. Co., Chicago. WOODS, F. A : 03. Mendel's laws and some records in rabbit breeding. Biometrika, vol. 2, pp. 299-306. 50 . "BRARY - in ; i •-• i • i , m • • ! ' '.' M • -• _ ;. • , 923 .,'•'-• . .-. . S S : S 55 j g ' •'•'•'•.'•;. . - - i