II- '•'•' 0 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION AND VALUE MARKING CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION AND VALUE MARKING BY J. C. MOTTRAM, M.B. LDN. • LONGMANS, GREEN AND CO. 39 PATERNOSTER ROW, LONDON NEW YORK, BOMBAY, AND CALCUTTA 1914 All rights reserved PREFACE AND INTRODUCTION THIS small volume makes public a new Theory. At the outset it is most important to make clear that no attempt is made to prove the Theory. For this reason it would have been better perhaps to have called it a New Hypothesis. Nevertheless, although proof is not attempted, the numerous ex- amples mentioned for purposes of illustration are, in themselves, facts which the Theory correlates, and therefore to some extent put the Theory to test. One reason why proof is not attempted, is that many and important observations which would best test the Theory, have either not been made or records of them have not been found. So much is this the case, that one of the chief reasons in bringing for- ward the Theory is to stimulate such research. Another reason is that it supports Charles Darwin's Causo-mechanical Theory of the Origin of Species. A weak part in this great Theory is and always has been the subsidiary 321924 vi CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION Theory of Sexual Selection. If therefore it could be shown that the origin of secondary sexual characters does not require this second Theory for their explanation, and if at the same time it can be shown that the origin of these characters is precisely similar to that of all characters : then the greater Theory, instead of being weakened by the Theory of Sexual Selection, will be strengthened by this widening of its capacity. This is the endeavour of the new Theory. It follows, therefore, that it has as a foun- dation— Darwin's Theory of the Origin of Species, and thus it has support : but it is also open to all the many arguments which are levelled against this foundation. The Theory does not deal only with secondary sexual characters, but also with very many others which cause di- or poly-morphisms. The Theory is described as much as possible in simple language ; technical words are when- ever possible avoided, in order that it may be intelligible to naturalists and field ob- servers, for it is their valuable work which is required for its substantiation or the re- verse. New terminologies are not introduced, although for descriptive purposes they would have been invaluable, because on account of PREFACE AND INTRODUCTION vii the hypothetical nature of the thesis, they did not appear to be justified. Repetition has been avoided as much as possible, even at the expense of forward reference, so that the whole volume must be read in order that a clear understanding may be obtained. The volume is divided in the following manner : Chapter I states the Theory; Chapters II, III, and IV deal at length with some facts on which the Theory rests ; the remaining chapters exemplify the Theory. Exception will probably be taken to some of the premises chosen, to some of the steps in the arguments ; but entrance into this most difficult subject is made only with great diffidence. Nevertheless hope is entertained that even if the Theory does not correlate a good number of the facts to which it is applied, it will at least explain a few. Even if it does no more than lead other minds along new lines of thought, the author will not be dis- satisfied. CONTENTS CHAP. PAGE PREFACE AND INTRODUCTION v I. THE THEORY 1 II. CONSPICUOUSNESS IN NATURE . . .12 III. CONSPICUOUS COLOUR . 21 IV. THE SELECTING POWER OF ENEMIES . . 42 V. FAMILY INSTINCTS 62 VI. THE COPULATORY ATTITUDES OF THE PAIR . 67 VII. PRE-COPULATORY DISPLAYS .... 73 VIII. SEXUAL COLOUR 78 IX. PARENTAL COLOUR . ... 91 X. PARENTAL AND SEXUAL CHARACTERS . . 98 XI. CONSIDERATION OF BRITISH BIRDS . 107 XII. HUMAN AND OTHER SOCIETIES . .122 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION AND VALUE MARKING CHAPTER I THE THEORY BEFORE statement of the Theory is given, certain facts of Nature must be called atten- tion to, upon which the Theory may be said to rest. Firstly, the individuals of a species are, in very many cases, not all of equal value. In the vast majority of sexually differentiated species, males are less valuable than females, for these reasons : — the soma carrying the female germ cells, the female, does not dis- charge its germ cells until they are fertilised, and often partially developed into embryos. Thus the female often is equivalent to male and female, and when about to give birth to young, to male and female in the form of young, individuals of the next generation. A 2, CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION On the other hand, the soma carrying the male germ cells, the male, discharges its germ cells before fertilisation, and therefore is at no time bisexual, as the female can be. For another reason, males are less valuable than females — because the male can fer- tilise a number of females ; therefore fewer males than females are required by a species. Further, as the sexes at birth commonly are approximately equal, and the environment of a species is always limited, and a species is best off when it fills its environment with individuals capable of the greatest fer- tility ... it follows that males, when in excess of females, or when equalling them in number, or perhaps even when somewhat less numerous, must be less valuable than females. Besides a difference in the value of individuals according to sex, there is a difference according to age. A very old and sterile, or relatively sterile, individual is not only valueless but harmful to the species, in that it fills a place that a fertile individual should occupy. Young are more valuable than old, because they have a longer life for reproduction before them. There are other factors which give in- dividuals of a species differences in value, THE THEORY 3 but sex and age are by far the most im- portant. The next fact that attention is drawn to, is that within a species individuals often differ widely in structure and characters. Males differ from females, young from old, in form, colour, structure, habits, instincts, and in many other characters apart from their sexual organs. These differences within a species are ap- parently often greater than the differences which distinguish one species from another (compare male and female pheasant with House and Tree sparrows), and are infinitely greater than the variations always to be found in every species, and by means of which, ac- cording to Darwin's theory of the Origin of Species, evolution has taken place. Attention to one other fact must be called, — namely, that individuals of a species often form themselves into societies : males and females become mated and form pairs, which keep together throughout the breeding season, or it may be for life. Parents and young also often keep together, forming families ; or families may meet to form herds ; societies which may be temporary or permanent. More complicated societies are not unknown ; for instance, the hive bees. 4 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION On these three facts the theory may be said to rest : 1. Within a species, individuals differ in value. 2. Within a species, individuals differ in structure. 3. Within a species, individuals form socie- ties. Further, there is a correlation between these facts, in that the differences in value are associated with the differences in structure. Females are more valuable than males ; females differ in structure from males other than in their sexual organs. Young are more valuable than old, and they too are different in structure. The third fact is also correlated, in that differ- ence in structure between male and female and between young and old are especially to be found in those animals which are closely united socially, either sexually or into families. The theory can now be stated under four headings : 1. Natural Selection appreciates the differ- ences in character which distinguish male from female, young from old. 2. Natural Selection must treat associations of individuals as units, just as it does single individuals. THE THEORY 5 3. Natural Selection, just as it brings about diversity of structure by acting on individuals, so it must bring about diversity of structure by acting upon associations of individuals. 4. These diversities of structure found in the unequally valuable members of societies, control Natural Selection in such a way that the less valuable are more liable to destruction than the more valuable. 1. If Darwin's theory of Natural Selection accounts for the evolution of organic matter, if Natural Selection appreciates the more or less small, continuous, or discontinuous varia- tions which occur among the individuals of a species, or even if it cannot appreciate these but only larger variations, mutations — if Natural Selection can do this, it must appreciate the relatively great differences which frequently distinguish male from female, old from young. If the scales of Natural Selection can detect a small difference be- tween two individuals, it must be able to easily detect great differences. If chance alone does not govern the selec- tion of very similar individuals, it is unlikely to do so when the individuals are very different. If the chance of taking one bird out of two hen birds is not half, because, 6 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION although apparently similar, they are in fact not so ; then the chance of selecting one out of male and female birds must also not be half; and in this case, because the individuals are markedly different in structure, it is likely to be further removed from half than in the case of the apparently similar hens. Thus these great differences in structure between male and female, young and old, must cause the male and the old to be more or less liable to be selected than the female and the young. One cannot conceive these individuals having an equal chance of survival in the struggle for existence. Continuing this conception further, a ques- tion at once arises : May not these differ- ences in structure between male and female, young and old, be such, that the more valuable females and young may have a better chance of survival than the less valuable males and old ? may not this be their purpose ? This question, although asked now, is answered under heading 4, and is best answered after headings 2 and 3 have been discussed. 2. Natural Selection must treat associations of individuals as units, just as it does indi- viduals. If, within a species, owing to the struggle for existence, the fittest individuals THE THEORY 7 alone survive — if the law of the survival of the fittest be true — then this law must also apply to pairs, families, or other societies of individuals, either temporary or permanent. If a pair A is better able to fit the environ- ment of the species than pair B, then pair A will have a better chance of survival, and so of reproducing themselves, than pair B. The male of pair A may not be better than the male of pair B, or the female of pair A than the female of pair B ; it is the combination that matters and not the individuals. So with all societies. If a community of bees A possess, as a variation, a character which enables them to better resist their enemies than a community B not possessing this varia- tion, then community A will survive and reproduce, whilst B will succumb. In fact, directly individuals form themselves into socie- ties, whatever be the bond, whether close or very slight, then they at once cease to be units re natural selection, re the survival of the fittest ; the society, the pair or the family at once becomes the unit. Males may be perhaps compared with males, females with females — this male may be said to be fitter than that ; but as long as the association lasts, an individual's mate is as much a part 8 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION of himself, as any of his personal characters, re evolution. Those structures or characters of the indi- viduals forming the society, which are alike, being common to all, can of course be con- sidered as units if desirable ; but those struc- tures or characters peculiar to, say, the male of a pair or of a family, cannot and must not be considered apart from the pair or the family. This conception has been carried thus far, in order that the explanation of heading 3 may be more easily understood. 3. Natural Selection, dc. — If the fixation of a useful variation in an individual is brought about by heredity and Natural Selection, then when a useful variation occurs in a society, although it may be confined to only one class of individuals and not be found in all the classes of individuals forming the society, it none the less will become fixed in a like manner. For instance, certain males of a species may develop, as a variation, a certain character which, although not beneficial to them, or it may even be harmful to them, is, however, useful to the pair, male and female ; enabling the pair to survive in the struggle for exist- THE THEORY 9 ence, and to reproduce itself, better than other pairs whose males do not possess this added character. Such a character, associated in the first place with maleness, will be handed down by the male to the pair's male offspring. Such a character should be looked upon as a character of the pair and not of the male, although bound to him. So with other societies, parents or young may possess, as variations, characters which enable the family to reproduce itself better than other families. When these become fixed they must be looked upon, not as characters of old individuals or young, but as family characters. It is possible to now explain heading 4. 4. These Diversities, &c. — If pairs, families, or other societies have characters, and if, within these societies, there are individuals of different value, then these societies must possess characters which compel Natural Selection to select the less valuable indi- viduals rather than the more valuable. Suppose a pair be subjected to selection, and suppose the male has, as variations, a character which causes the selecting agent to destroy him, the less valuable, rather than the female, the more valuable ; then this pair will 10 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION have an advantage over other pairs, in which the chance of male and female surviving is equal. If the female be destroyed, the character of the female and the male will not be repro- duced ; at any rate the chance of his, the male, reproducing himself is less than males in general, for he must find another mate. If the male be destroyed, it by no means follows that the male characters will be de- stroyed, for it is likely that he has already fertilised the female, especially as the society, a pair in the majority of animals, only occurs during the breeding season. Directly the percentage of males in the population become much reduced polygamy results, and the value of the male will rise until it equals that of the female : directly this balance is struck, further evolution of the male, making him more liable to be selected, will cease. Similarly with any other society of unequally valuable individuals, they will become marked according to their value, and in such a way that Natural Selection will be controlled and compelled to take the less valuable rather than the more valuable. A review of the exposition. Facts : 1. Individuals of a species not of equal value : male and female, old and young. 2. Individuals form themselves into so- cieties : pairs, families, herds, &c. THE THEORY 11 3. Individuals differ widely in structure : male from female, young from old. 4. Correlation of these three facts : unequal value with difference of structure, and both with the formation of societies. Sequence of argument : 1. Natural Selection must appreciate differ- ences in structure ; 2. Natural Selection must treat associations as units ; 3. And bring about diversity of structure in them ; 4. Which structures control Natural Selec- tion, so that less valuable individuals are more liable to be destroyed than more valuable. Thus when male is found to be different from female it is necessary to inquire whether the difference be due to (1) a character of the individual which fits a monosexual en- vironment, or (2) a character of the pair, also of cause an environmental character, that controls Natural Selection. And so with all structures occurring in sociable animals and confined to particular individuals. Thus it is as a working hypothesis that this new theory is brought forward, in the hope that it may possibly explain the origin and use of some polymorphisms. CHAPTER II CONSPICUOUSNESS IN NATURE BEFORE proceeding to exemplify the Theory, it is necessary, as will appear later, to con- sider the methods by which animals render themselves purposely conspicuous. Many ways are utilised ; they may be conveniently described under the following headings : Move- ment, Form, Position, Colour, Sound, Scent. Conspicuous Movement. — It is freely ad- mitted that, in Nature, moving animals are more conspicuous than resting ones ; the more perfect the rest, the less conspicuous is the animal. The simulation of death by the sus- pension of every movement is commonly employed by animals for protection, and ex- amples are to be found among every class of animals. The majority, the vast majority of animals, must perform various movements for the ob- taining of food, for reproduction and other vital processes. These motions are necessary, 12 CONSPICUOUSNESS IN NATURE 13 in spite of the fact that animals thus render themselves conspicuous to their enemies. Means are usually taken to make them as little conspicuous as possible ; they are carried out under cover, or at night, or only after inspection has proved the absence of any enemy. At the slightest alarm they cease. It is not to this kind of motion that at- tention is now to be drawn, but to those motions made by animals whose purpose is to make themselves conspicuous. Motions of this kind, purposely conspicuous, are made chiefly for two reasons : (1) to signal to friends ; (2) to signal to enemies. Signals to friends give notice of danger, of security, of the presence of a comrade, of the pre- sence of a male or female, and of many other things. It is not to these signals, but to the signals to enemies, that attention is now directed. Signals to enemies are of two kinds, and made for two reasons : (1) Repellent, to repel the enemy, to frighten it away ; (2) Attrac- tive, to attract the enemy, to draw its attack. Examples of these two kinds of signals by motion are to be found widely distributed throughout the animal kingdom. The majority of animals at bay attempt to frighten away an 14 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION enemy by making some menacing movement ; even quite small animals, insects for instance, will then make threatening motions. Under Eepellent motions would also be classed Warn- ing motions ; those which presuppose know- ledge on the part of the enemy, the attacker ; a knowledge gained by previous association of the motion with some unpleasantness. There is considerable evidence that this kind of motion is frequently mimicked. These signals by movement are, as a rule, associated with numerous other displays — displays of position, attitude, colour, sound, and scent ; for this reason they are difficult to analyse. It is often difficult to decide whether the motion is the essential or an accessory of the signal. (2) Attractive. — Motion signals, whose pur- pose is to attract an enemy, although fre- quently overlooked, are also common in Nature. Amongst birds, as mentioned in Chapters V and X under Family Instincts, parents will frequently endeavour, by the making of conspicuous movements, to attract an enemy, to draw its attack. Numerous other examples will be given when considera- tion is made of Sexual Displays. Purposely conspicuous movement is probably, therefore, CONSPICUOUSNESS IN NATURE 15 an entity in Nature, and may be conveniently classified thus : 1. Signals to friends. Purposely conspicuous movements. I 2. Signals to enemies. (a) Repelling conspicuous move- ment : to repel an enemy, to frighten it away. (&) Attracting conspicuous movement : to attract an enemy, to draw its attack. Position. — Conspicuousness as the result of position cannot properly be considered apart from the animal's surroundings, their form and colour. A moth resting on a tree- trunk may be conspicuous or not, according as its form and colour contrasts or harmonises with that of the tree. However, there is little doubt that some animals sometimes, and others frequently, purposely take up positions which render them conspicuous in Nature. It is not always easy to decide whether any given observed position of an animal which is conspicuous is the result of accident or purpose. Only by a thorough knowledge of the habits of the animal, and after many observations have been made, can this be decided. For in every animal's surroundings can be found that which will render it, on the one hand, conspicuous, or 16 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION on the other, inconspicuous. The form, colour, and habits of the animal may be such that it is seldom or never found among sur- roundings which render it inconspicuous ; or its form, colour, and habits may produce the opposite effect ; unfortunately our knowledge of the habits of animals is so small (possibly because our attention has been chiefly con- centrated on the study of their other charac- ters) that we are usually unable to decide whether the observed position in Nature of an animal is conspicuous or inconspicuous, the result of accident or purpose. Further analysis of this method will not be gone into, especially as it must again be frequently referred to when dealing with other methods. Form. — By form, animals can be and are more readily recognised than by any other attribute ; in fact, it is by the amount of space they occupy that we know them. There are, no doubt, species which are more easily distinguished from closely allied species by colour or some other character, because they are alike in form and size ; but the mind, unconsciously, first places them according to their form. A wasp is recognised, first by its shape, CONSPICUOUSNESS IN NATURE 17 but afterwards the mind may decide what species of wasp it is by its colour, or some other character. For this reason, form per se is a character which the majority of animals endeavour to hide, because the majority of animals wish to make themselves inconspicuous in Nature. So much is this so, that a study of protection is in truth a study of the means used to obliterate form. Nevertheless, some animals which are pur- posely conspicuous in Nature take no pre- cautions to obliterate form, but, in fact, rather accentuate their shape ; these may be said to possess purposely conspicuous form. Thayer has shown that form is commonly rendered inconspicuous by obliterative shading. Absence of obliterative shading or anti-obliterative shading, when found in animals whose form could be concealed by obliterative shading, will be examples of purposely conspicuous form. Purposely conspicuous form may be classi- fied like purposely conspicuous motion, ac- cording as the signal is to friends or to enemies. Mention has already been made of con- spicuousness, produced by the taking up of particular positions ; this may be accentuated by the assumption of some easily seen form in B 18 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION that position. Animals in defence, in order to beat off an enemy, will erect combs, feathers, or hairs, will open mouths, stretch necks, take up an upright position, and in many other ways assume conspicuous attitudes ; these may be called Eepelling Conspicuous Atti- tudes. Just as there are attracting conspicuous movements, so there are attracting conspicuous attitudes. Many animals by thus making them- selves conspicuous endeavour to draw an enemy's attack, for instance, from their young. Birds especially will erect combs, spread tails, and assume all manner of conspicuous attitudes for this purpose. A large number of pre- copulatory displays consist in the assumption of conspicuous attitudes. Sound. — Sound is seldom used for ought but signalling to friend or foe. Silence spells concealment, sound conspicuousness. Sound can be similarly classified : Purposely conspicuous ^ sounds. 1. Signals to ((«) Between the aex* - . , i (o) Between members or other I societies. 2. Signals to enemies. (a.) Repellent sounds : to enemy away. (b) Attractive (conspicuous) frighten an (conspicuous) sounds : to attract an enemy, to draw its attack. CONSPICUOUSNESS IN NATURE 19 Repellent sounds are very commonly used to ward off a foe : sudden and therefore startling sounds are especially used. Attract- ing sounds, to draw an enemy's attack from the young, are often difficult to distinguish from the alarm notes made to warn the young of danger ; it may be that sometimes the same note is used for the two purposes. The cry of the Plover or Curlew certainly causes the young to take cover, and more than likely distracts the enemy's attention from the young to the screaming parents. Evidence will be brought forward later to show that the songs of birds are to protect the female, by drawing the attention of any near predatory animal to the singer, the male. It may be here mentioned that the drawing of an enemy's attack is not in any way an act of self-destruction. Predatory animals usually take their victims by surprise, and probably then stand as good a chance of making a successful capture, as when the victim is purposely exposing itself in order to draw attack. Scent. — Scent is used precisely like Sound. Repellent scents are always unpleasant ; but owing to the inferiority of our sense of smell, 20 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION knowledge of the subject is meagre, and prob- ably untrustworthy. It may be that there are pleasing scents, the purpose of which is to attract enemies. One other method, by means of which animals render themselves conspicuous, re- mains to be considered, namely colour. This has been left to the last, because it is the most important and because a previous con- sideration of the other methods may render its consideration more comprehensible. This is dealt with in the next chapter. CHAPTER III CONSPICUOUS COLOUR IT is not possible to conceive that the colour of an animal, in Nature, is purposeless, indif- ferent, the result of chance. The colour of domesticated animals may be conceived to be of such a nature, but if one believes in the Origin of Species as expounded by Charles Darwin, then the colour of animals in Nature must be looked upon as purposeful ; and it can have but two uses : (1) to make an animal inconspicuous, (2) to make it conspicuous. This it can only do by contrast ; to consider the colour of an animal per se must be meaning- less ; only the comparison of its colour with that of its surroundings can be of any value. Therefore when considering the colour of animals, at least three factors must be taken into account : (1) the colour of the animal, (2) the colour of its environments, and (3) the animal's habits. Without a knowledge of its habits, it is impossible to decide whether an 21 22 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION animal is conspicuous or inconspicuous in Nature, wherein all kinds of backgrounds are to be found, backgrounds which will render the most brilliantly coloured animal incon- spicuous, and backgrounds which will make the dullest easy to see. For this reason, the animal's habits must be taken into account ; it is necessary to know whether a given animal is commonly or uncommonly seen against obliterating backgrounds. It is also necessary to know, for instance, in the case of butterflies, what are the common back- grounds found behind the insects on the one hand when they have open wings, and on the other, when they have closed. Colour rarely is used for other purposes. Metabolic processes may give rise to colours which are purposeless, apart from metabolism. Waste products which have to be got rid of, may be deposited on the surfaces of plants and animals. Colour may have other uses ; for instance, black for the absorption of the sun's heat, white for its reflection : the green chlorophyl of plants comes under this category. Eeference will again be made to Habits, when Thayer's theory (that all animals are protectively coloured) is being criticised ; in the meantime, apart from habits, certain generalisations of colour in Nature can be made. CONSPICUOUS COLOUR 23 The face of our earth is, broadly speaking, coloured with secondary colours, greens, browns, and greys ; these colours are not laid down in flat tints but in broken masses, which run one into the other, and nowhere have defined margins. Individual leaves and single stones can only be distinguished at close range, and are then seen never to be homogeneously coloured. Certain portions of the globe are not so coloured. The Arctic regions, mountain tops, and sub-arctic regions in the winter are covered with an even cloak of white ; deserts are evenly coloured light brown ; oceans grey or blue ; sky blue. Into these backgrounds many animals fade, through similarity of colouring. Spotted coats and barred plumes of brown, green, and grey blend with the field and forest ; white animals in the snow, sandy-coloured in the desert, grey at sea, &c., are invisible from afar. Against these backgrounds certain parts of plants and many animals stand out in marked contrast — how are they coloured ? In an exactly opposite manner : instead of being coloured with secondary colours, the primary ones are used, red, yellow, blue, with black and white ; and instead of the colour 24 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION being laid down in broken masses, it is painted on in broad flat tints. In this way the maximum contrast is obtained. Flowers are purposely conspicuous, so that insects and birds may see and visit them ; they are coloured with even washes of the primary colours and with white. White daisies, yellow buttercups, blue cornflowers, red poppies, are examples always to hand ; green, grey, or brown flowers form a very small percentage of the whole ; they as a rule do not desire the visits of the hoipolloi of insects, but of one particular insect which they attract by scent or some other method, apart from colour. The efficiency of this method of making themselves conspicuous in Nature is patent to everyone who views wild flowers ; they will not be passed by unseen ; on the other hand, brown and green flowers require diligent searching for, with a well- trained eye. The same may be said of edible fruits which present purposely conspicuous colouring ; they are found to be coloured with the primary colours and with black and white, laid down evenly. Turn now to animals. Many are undoubtedly protectively coloured with mottled greens, CONSPICUOUS COLOUR 25 greys, and browns ; but there are others which are painted with broad masses of the primary colours and with black and white. May not these present, like fruit and flowers, seeing that the same method is used, purposeful con- spicuous colouring ? A more detailed analysis of these colours will now be made. White. — In regions where snow, either tem- porarily or permanently, covers the ground, white is used cryptically or protectively. It is used also on the under- sides of many animals, to produce obliterative modelling, as Thayer has shown. Thayer is of opinion, too, that it may also be used to conceal an animal against the sky ; but when a flat white surface is held against the sky at different angles, it will either appear much darker than, or if the sun is out, much lighter than the white of the sky and be very conspicuous ; only within a very small angle will it be equal in tone to the sky. Further, only a very small portion of the sky can be thus matched, away from the sun and not far above the horizon. Thus it appears that only with care, and in one particular position, can a FLAT white surface be made to appear inconspicuous against the sky : with a solid white object, 26 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION it is of course quite impossible. A swan, for instance, must always be conspicuous against the sky. White, therefore, except in snow and to produce obliterative modelling, will render an animal conspicuous in Nature. Black. — As with white, it is possible to conceive of a black animal being invisible, for instance, against a black shadow : a rook sitting in the innermost recess of a thick forest would be difficult to see ; but black shadows are almost unknown in Nature, especially cold black, such as is found in many animals. Shadows in Nature are invariably warm, because the objects in them are lit by the reflected light from surrounding warmly coloured objects. Animals coloured black, and especially cold black, must therefore be looked upon as con- spicuously coloured. Attention had again better be called to the fact, that it is not enough to study colour alone ; habits must also be taken into account. A black animal, living in dark forests and always shunning the light, cannot be compared in conspicuousness with a black animal which lives in open country, e.g. the female Blackbird with the Starling or Rook. Black therefore must be looked upon as CONSPICUOUS COLOUR 27 conspicuous, unless contradicted by the habits of the animal. Blue. — Blue, in Nature, is found in the sky and in its reflection in water and snow. If white animals are seldom, if ever, inconspicuous against the white (light grey) of the sky, then blue animals can never be in harmony with the blue of the sky. The amount of blue light coming from a blue sky is enormous compared to the amount coming from an object coloured even very light blue, and placed in the brightest sunlight. It is therefore impossible for a terrestrial animal coloured blue (a blue butterfly for instance) ever to be in harmony with a blue sky. Blue, used protectively, is however found in oceanic fishes inhabiting the surface of the water ; as a rule their dorsal surfaces are coloured blue, in harmony with the low- toned blue of very deep water as it is seen from above. Now the blue of these fish is not less brilliant than the blue found in terrestrial animals ; and as this blue harmonises with the low-toned blue of the sea, it cannot, in terrestrial animals, ever harmonise with the intensely high-toned blue of the sky. It thus appears that the blue of terrestrial animals is not protective ; and if not protective, it must 28 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION be purposely conspicuous, as it cannot be indifferent. Yellow. — The primary colour, yellow, is a very rare environment for animals, and so it is very rarely used protectively. A few insects which fly in the autumn are yellow, in harmony with the yellow autumnal foliage. Sunlight penetrating young green foliage is not a pure colour but yellow-green ; this colour is found on the breasts of a number of birds, the Willow Wren (Phylloscopus trochilus) for in- stance ; compare this with the yellow bar in the Goldfinch's (Carduelis elegans) wing, and with the yellow of the male Golden Oriole. Red. — Above all others, animals coloured red are the most conspicuous in Nature, because it is never found in the surroundings. The rising and setting sun sometimes colours all things red, animals as well as their sur- roimdings. Thayer is of opinion that some scarlet animals, Flamingoes (Phoenicopterus ruber} for instance, are coloured thus to render them invisible against the red of the setting sun ; that which has already been said about sky protections, under White and Blue, also applies to Red ; a red animal against the setting sun, by contrast, looks not red but black. CONSPICUOUS COLOUR 29 Having analysed to some extent colour in Nature, and shown that some colours cannot, and others probably do not, render an animal inconspicuous, and must therefore make it conspicuous, it now becomes necessary to consider the colour and habits of animals conjointly. By means of examples it will be shown that the colour and habits of animals are correlated ; and that conspicuous colouring in animals is a fact of Nature. The Sheldrake (Tadorna cornuta) will be the first example : the sexes are nearly similar, coloured with large masses of black, white, and red ; it lives in estuaries, on mud flats and sand banks, it nests in sand hills ; among these surroundings, no matter at what angle it is viewed, it is an object visible from afar, often at a distance of several miles. It is possible to conceive of a background, of boulders for instance, or of different coloured seaweeds on white sand, against which the bird would not be conspicuous : just as it is easy to conceive of a background against which it would be conspicuous : but ask the wild-fowler or longshoreman which is the most conspicuous bird on the shore, and he will say— the Sheldrake. Systematically watch the birds over a long period of time 30 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION and from different heights, from the tops of cliffs as well as from the bases, not once in a day will they become difficult to see. If the Sheldrake is protectively coloured, and this is the only alternative, then why does it utilise a different colouring from the other shore birds, which are undoubtedly protec- tively coloured — the Waders, &c. ? If, on the other hand, it is conspicuously coloured, how does it manage to hold its own against hawks and other predatory animals ? By living always in open country where it cannot be approached unawares, by always being close to water where it can seek safety by diving. Why is it conspicuously coloured ? Although to show the raison d'etre of con- spicuous colouring would add much to the above argument, this must be reserved for a future chapter, in order to prevent confusion and overlapping of the subject. The next example chosen is the >Stonechat (Pratincola Rubicola), a bird presenting a very similar colour to the Sheldrake — black, red, and white. These are its habits. It lives and breeds on open commons, always a con- spicuous bird from its habit of constantly perching on the topmost branches of the CONSPICUOUS COLOUR 31 furze bushes. Stonechats have been observed for many hours at a stretch and from all angles, but only on very rare occasions are they difficult to see, as when they have dropped down for a moment on to broken ground after some insect food. They never depart far from their undergrowth of furze or briar, so have constantly at hand a secure refuge in case of attack, and thus do not require protective coloration. The usual background of this bird is the sky and dark bushes, not broken ground. Consider the Rook (C. Frugilegus). In the early morning the flock leave the woods, to spend the whole day in the open country, where they are a mark for miles around, especially on a day of sun, for then the flashes of light from their glossy plumages will reveal their presence three or four miles away. In a tree the bird is equally visible as a dark mass against the sky. When it is possible to look down on a colony of their nests, the sitting birds stand out blue-black in marked contrast to the broken brown of the nests. The Eook is a social bird : there is safety in numbers : it will be an advantage to a single lost bird to be able to see the flock from afar. 32 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION The Soldier Beetle (Telephones) is con- spicuous in Nature, not entirely because it is coloured black and red, but because it chooses to spend its days on the surfaces of white umbelliferous flowers, whence it can be seen from afar and is at the mercy of any bird who cares to feed upon it. The Black Beetle (Pimarcha Icevigata) is coloured an intense, cold, blue-black. It is a beetle which is at large during the day, crawling slugglishly and clumsily about and taking no pains to conceal itself : a very con- spicuous object. If given to birds, they will either not take them, or having picked at them will spend many minutes scraping their beaks, obviously in an endeavour to get rid of a very unpleasant flavour ; they will seldom swallow these beetles. The Soldier Beetle is similarly treated. The Cinnabar Moth (Euchelia Jacobcece). Coloured black and vivid red : it flies by day : it is a slow flier and would fall an easy victim to any enemy : it never takes any pains to conceal itself, as other insects do when dis- turbed : it does not bury itself in the grasses : it does not seek safety in flight. The Large White Butterfly (P. brassicce). A slow flier — it can be seen at least 500 yards CONSPICUOUS COLOUR 33 away — it would fall an easy victim to any bird. In London, where the Sparrows have had little experience of White Butterflies, I have on several occasions (four) seen Sparrows (P. domesticus) take them on the wing, but always, after pecking them and killing them, they have left them. Apparently they are relatively unpalatable. The Red Admiral Butterfly (Pyrameis Atalanta) spends many hours of the day, with outstretched wings, on flowers : flat flowers are especially chosen ; here it is always a conspicuous object, no matter how viewed. It depends for its safety on quickness of sight and rapidity of flight. When sleeping, it closes its wings, displaying the under surfaces only, which are not conspicuously coloured ; and it chooses to rest among surroundings which render it inconspicuous — among dead leaves or on the barks of trees, &c. If disturbed whilst thus resting, it does not fly away, but relies for escape entirely on its pro- tective colouring, and the simulation of an inanimate object. These habits associated in the same insect, with exposure of the bright upper surfaces of the wings on the one hand, and with the dull under surfaces on the other, indicate, c 34 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION without doubt, that the upper surfaces are conspicuous in Nature and the under ones inconspicuous. It is probably an advantage for Admiral butterflies to be able to see other Admiral butterflies on flowers, from a distance ; and thus to find good food and mates, &c. The Tadpole (Rana Temporaries), when very young, is coloured uniformly black. At this stage, they keep together in colonies, forming black wriggling masses, as noticeable from under water as from above, and make, when disturbed, no effort at concealment. It is noteworthy that the black of the young tadpole, like the black of the frog's ovum, may be for the purpose of absorbing the sun's heat. Experiments have shown that young tad- poles are unpalatable to both birds and fish ; it seems, therefore, that they can with safety render themselves conspicuous, and utilise the beneficial heat-absorbing property of blackness. Sufficient examples, and they could be multiplied almost indefinitely, have now been given, to show that many animals are con- spicuous in Nature and make no attempt to conceal themselves. CONSPICUOUS COLOUR 35 It appears, therefore, that Thayer's theory, that all animals are protectively coloured, does not correctly describe the facts. An artist's eye will note, or his hand be able to depict, the most brilliant animals among surround- ings which render them inconspicuous ; but if the animals are rarely found among these surroundings, then such observations and such illustrations appertain to exceptions and not to the rule, and thereby hide the truth. Certain broad conclusions may now be made : 1. Animals which are coloured with the primary colours and with black and white, and on which these colours are laid down in large and distinct areas, MAY or may NOT present conspicuous colouring : a close study of their habits and surroundings can alone decide how they must be classed. 2. Animals which are coloured with the secondary colours, or with the primary colours much broken into small masses, one into the other, are almost invariably protectively coloured. But in these also, habits must be taken into account. One is now in a position to classify colour ; as before mentioned, it must first be divided into Purposely Con- spicuous and Purposely Inconspicuous. These 36 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION may be again sub-divided as in the following table : < COLOUR inconspicuous to enemies protective = procryptic. to food aggressive = anticryptic. (to friends) (? valueless.) conspicuous to signal to enemies to attract enemies = attracting colour. to repel enemies = Aposematic. = Pseudo-sematic. to signal to friends Recognition marks = Episematic. to signal to food to attract food (as in Mantidae) = Pseudo-episematic. = alluring colour. The colour of an animal can, however, never be considered as a unit. Some of the coloured parts will be used for one purpose, others for another. For instance, in many animals and especially in birds, parts are conspicuously coloured which are usually concealed, and which are only displayed on definite occa- sions and for definite purposes : thus, too, the Red Admiral butterfly can, at will, display CONSPICUOUS COLOUR 37 the upper surface of its wings, or the under ones. No general statement, therefore, can be made of its coloration, but its upper and under surfaces must be separately labelled. Further, it is readily conceived that additional knowledge of its habits might necessitate qualifications of these labellings : for instance, the black might prove to be a signal to enemies, and the red bar a social signal. Here, however, colour must be considered only in so far as it is purposely conspicuous, and especially with regard to signals to enemies. Signals to enemies are of two kinds only : (a) in order to repel enemies ; (6) in order to attract enemies. Examples of repelling conspicuous colouring (conspicuous colouring associated with un- palatableness) are afforded by animals which have warning coloration : it is unnecessary to give examples, but it may be mentioned that, in the opinion of many, protection is not obtained, in the vast majority of these cases, by an association in the minds of predatory animals, of unpleasantness with a particular species ; but with a particular class of animals, coloured similarly. For instance, a bird will pass by a black beetle, not because it has tasted that particular 38 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION species, but because it is coloured black like another species, whose flavour it has sampled ; indeed the first beetle may in reality be palat- able. Thus have been accounted for many cases of mimicry, and their origin, by fortuitous variations of palatable insects resembling un- palatable. To describe a colour as a warning colour, is to describe it from the point of view of an enemy ; it is a description of purpose rather than of effect. The following table, given in Professor Poulton's book, is a classification of colour for the most part from this point of view. Attracting colour has not been intro- duced : it would fall under B after some modification. The terminologies here used have been placed in their appropriate places in the table given on p. 36. A. Apatetic Colours = colours resembling some part of environment. (1) Cryptic (a) procryptic = protective resemblance. (b) anticryptic = aggressive resemblance. (2) Pseudo-sematic = false signalling. (a) pseudo-sematic = protective mimicry. (b) pseudo-episematic = aggressive mimi- cry or alluring. B. Sematic Colours = signalling colours. (1) Aposematic = warning. (2) Episematic = recognition marks. [(3) 1 = attracting colour.] CONSPICUOUS COLOUR 39 Eeturning to Table I, Repelling Conspicu- ous Colouring is also exemplified by Alarm- ing Coloration ; many birds will frighten away an enemy by suddenly displaying vivid colour — the distention of scarlet bladders, or erection of bright plumes. Insects also utilise this method of scaring enemies ; organs coloured with the primary colours are suddenly disclosed or protruded. Attracting Conspicuous Colouring, it will be shown, is possibly much commoner in Nature than is generally supposed ; accepted examples are as yet uncommon. The bright under- wings of many insects are supposed to be of this nature, that predatory animals may seize them, rather than more vulnerable parts. Parent birds, when protecting their young, will display their brightest plumes, in order to attract the enemy's attention, to draw its attack ; and many examples are given in Chapter VIII. In the meantime, mention may be made of certain colour marks, here called attraction marks. The eye-spots found in insects are con- sidered to be for the purpose of attracting an enemy's attention to them in order that 40 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION it may be induced to strike them rather than more vulnerable parts, which have not eye- spots. Other marks and colourings serve the same purpose ; for instance, the brilliant under- wings of many moths. Here are examples of purposely conspicuous characters whose function is to attract enemies. Whenever conspicuous colouring is found on invulnerable parts, the question must be asked, Is this an attracting mark ? There is some evidence that these marks are by no means uncommon. In birds, for instance, the con- spicuous marks and bars on wings and tail may be of this nature. As is often the case in insects, they are concealed when at rest and displayed during flight, and may cause enemies to strike at tail or wing and thus not injure vital parts. It is noteworthy that although young as a rule do not present conspicuous colouring, they do usually have these attraction marks. (For examples see Chapter IX.) It will be well to review the conclusions which have been arrived at in the last two chapters : 1. Purposeful conspicuousness of animals is a fact of Nature. 2. It is produced in animals by means of CONSPICUOUS COLOUR 41 movement, form, position, sound, scent, and colour ; and probably in other ways. 3. It is used for three purposes : A, to signal to friends ; B, to signal to enemies ; and C, rarely, to signal to food. 4. The signals to enemies are of two kinds : A, to repel them ; B, to attract them. CHAPTEE IV THE SELECTING POWER OF ENEMIES IT is impossible to conceive of an environ- ment, without also calling to mind that which is invested. A biological environment is the sum of many factors which, added together, permit of life. A study of these factors shows that they are not homogeneous, but that each of them is made up of a continuous series of diversities. Many obviously have this constitution, such as temperature, alti- tude above sea-level, the relation of the differ- ent points on a tree-trunk to the points of the compass. Others seem to form a discontin- uity, but in these cases steps are probably not absent, only crowded together. Just as environment cannot be conceived alone, so living matter cannot : the two are inextricably mingled. The structure of living matter is bound to its function : and function is action on environment. Without environ- ment, living matter could not be recognised. The Environment of a Unit of Living THE SELECTING POWER OF ENEMIES 43 Matter. — Here, a unit of living matter is used in the sense of a single individual, one of many which, collectively, form a species. If examination be made of its environment, no matter which factor be chosen for observation, it will be found to be definitely bounded, in spite of the factor not being homogeneous. During a definite time — this must be taken into account — a plant occupies a definite quan- tity of space, it takes from the soil a definite quantity of mineral matter and receives a definite amount of sunlight. A single animal wandering on a mountain- side, climbs up and down to definite heights above sea-level. This power of being able to adapt itself to the diversities of its environment, within limits, is a property of all living matter. The Environment of a Species. — If the en- vironment of a unit is defined, then the en- vironment of a collection of units, a species, must be definite. In the case of sexual dimorphism and other di- and poly-morphisms, the environment of the species would be the environments of the several types of individuals added together : the defined environment of the male added to the defined environment of the female, &c. This addition is necessary because the environment of the several types of individuals is not identical, although each is defined. 44 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION Fig. 1 is a pictorial representation of this conception ; ~EV E2, E3, &c., are the factors of the environment, shaded, to show their serial nature ; the area enclosed by dark lines is the environment of the species. Fig. 2 is a simplified illustration. The environment of E.i. E.2. E.3. E.4. E.s. E.e. FIG. 1. FIG. 2. a species can be classified into common and specific. The common environment is shared with other species (many animals breathe air and drink water) ; its specific environment it occupies, solus. If, as has been considered, structure means THE SELECTING POWER OF ENEMIES 45 environment, it is impossible to conceive of two species occupying the same environment : because one cannot conceive of two species which, being different in structure, are yet similar in function ; which is the same as having similar relations to the same environ- ment. Different structures, different functions, different environments — here is an analogy ; but in different structures, similar functions, and the same environment — there is none. Allied species, or a species and an estab- lished variety of it, may occupy neighbouring environments, but never the same. If allied species do not, distant species probably cannot. Thus a species must be alone in its specific environment, and never the subject of com- petition with other species. On account of the absence of competition and because a species increases in geometrical progression, the species must always completely fill its specific environment ; except when it has been unable to keep pace with an increase in quantity of its specific environment. A species is rare or common, according to whether its specific environment is restricted or unconfined. As will be shown later, it is not possible to conceive of a restricted common environment, and a surplus of specific environment. 46 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION The Relation of Species to Species. — The environment of one species may form that of another — beneath forest trees, tree-ferns thrive ; on the stems of tree-ferns, filmy ferns find suitable homes. The cat feeds on the mouse, whose enemy the cat is. Thus, one species can, through its environment, affect that of another ; but a species, per se, cannot affect another species because action implies something acted upon, and anything upon which a species acts is its environment. Species may occupy a common environment, but, as will be shown, in so doing, neither each other nor their specific environments are affected. fc». The Species and its Environment. Their Relations. — So far, the following conceptions have been arrived at. Structure equals en- vironment. The boundaries of the specific environment of a species are defined. Each species has its own specific environment. A species completely fills its specific environ- ment. A species can only be acted upon by physical and biological forces, through its environment. Further consideration must now be made of the selecting agent, the Environment. It has been shown not to be homogeneous, but THE SELECTING POWER OF ENEMIES 47 that all of its factors are parts of a continuous series. Therefore the selecting power of the environment will not be homogeneous but will be arranged through the environment, in the form of a continuous series. And as the individuals that go to form a species are not identical, but differ more or less from each, it follows that these variations of a species, E. i. E. 2. E. 3. E. 4. E. 3. - ^- jj _ V. i. V. 2. V. 3. V. 4. V. 8. _ V. 1 " J ^* IK FIG. 3. whether continuous or discontinuous, will TEND to become serially arranged through the specific environment, according to the serial arrangement of the selecting power of the environment : as is shown in Fig. 3, Vv V2, V3-Vn perfectly fitting Ep E2, E3-En. If discontinuous there will be gaps. In Nature, this is prevented by inter- crossing (and migration). If the species were 48 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION distributed over an unlimited space, then the environment would be differentiated; but because, in Nature, space is always limited, this environmental classification only occurs to a limited extent ; when a species is dis- tributed over a wide area, when the environ- mental gradient is very slight, then this establishment of varieties and sub-species may take place locally. The above use of such words as SPACE, LOCALLY, AREA, &c. may give the impression that distribution through space is alone alluded to : these words are used with refer- ence to the environmental gradient, whatever be the factors of its composition. SPACE is chosen for illustration, for clear- ness' sake. Varieties and sub-species may thus become established by environmental differentiation, and, it may be, evoluted into distinct species. Thus, without the help of any outside force, new species may arise within the enclosure of a specific environment, as in Fig. 4. In a similar way, the environment may become sexually differentiated when the sexes are separate, as in Fig. 5. Males often differ widely in structure from females, and occupy an entirely different environment. This sexual THE SELECTING POWER OF ENEMIES 49 differentiation of the specific environment is the only possible cause of secondary sexual characters, as this volume attempts to show. Although other theories, as for instance Darwin's theory of Sexual Selection, attempt to account for these structural differences in other ways. Also, in polymorphic species, the environment becomes similarly split up. !' • I I. 1 , LI, FIG. 4. O. FIG. 5. It has been seen that the specific environ- ment is curtailed by the limit of the ability of the species to adapt itself to the diversities of the factors which form its environment. This property is common to all living matter, and without it Life, as we know it, could not exist. If living matter could not thus accom- modate itself but could only occupy a homo- D 50 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION geneous environment, then living matter would assume an infinite number of different forms, corresponding to the infinite number of the diversities of its environment ; and each form would be infinitely restricted (see Fig. 3). Associated with this adaptation must be structural alteration ; function and structure are inseparable — these are acquired char- acters. Nevertheless, in spite of this power of adap- tation, one cannot conceive a variation of a species, whatever its nature, displacing another species from its specific environment, which it completely fills and perfectly fits ; to accom- plish this, the variation would have to differ from the parent in a great number of struc- tures, be a great mutation and would have at the same time to fit perfectly a new environment, in fact more perfectly than the former occupier, which itself is a perfect fit ; can such a correlation come about through Chance ? Change in Environment. — Change in en- vironment may be quantitative ; the specific environment may increase, decrease, or dis- appear entirely, in which case the species will becomejcommon, rare, or extinct. The change may be qualitative, and consist in the addi- THE SELECTING POWER OF ENEMIES 51 tion or subtraction of one or more factors to or from the specific environment. The subtraction of a factor may lead to extinction of the species ; a food supply may fail. Or if the species continues to thrive, then those structures which were related to the lost factor will degenerate. Those indi- viduals who waste the least nourishment or sustenance on a useless organ will have an advantage over others in which this organ is more developed. Degenerated organs are found in species occupying degenerated en- vironments. Degenerate structures will be found to vary most because of the absence of the fixing power of environmental selection. If a factor be subtracted locally, then a local degenerate variety or sub-species will be found. The addition of a new factor to the specific environments may produce extinction. Short of extinction, an added factor will prevent a species fitting its environment perfectly ; but sooner or later, as the result of variation of the species and environmental selection, new structures will arise which will enable the species to perfectly fit the new environ- ment. Further addition of factors will be followed by the addition of new structures ; 52 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION in this way, there will be evolution. Highly evolved species will be found in highly com- plex and often restricted environments ; such species will be found to vary very little. High specialisation demands great fixity of structure. The local addition of a factor will produce local evolution and the estab- lishment of a sub-species. Reasons have been given why one cannot conceive of a species being displaced from its specific environment by the variety of another : for similar reasons, one cannot conceive of a variation of a species being able either to occupy an environment rendered vacant by the extinction of a species, or to discover an entirely new environment. Evolution equals environmental differentiation, the better ap- preciation by organic matter of the diversities of its environment. The question of com- petition yet remains to be considered. As has already been shown, this can only occur within a common environment, and then only when the common environment is not sufficient to accommodate the two or more species which share it. Suppose (see Fig. 6) Sx and S2 are two species, El and E2 are their specific environments, and E3 a common environment, where competition is THE SELECTING POWER OF ENEMIES 53 going on. Then the common environment will be a selecting agent and will select either Sj or S2 ; one or other must be selected, for they cannot both be alike re the environment. The selected one will then include the common environment in its specific environment, as in Fig. 7. If they are very similar, then different parts of the environment having slightly different selecting powers, because of its diversity, will select, some S1? some S2. FIG. 6. The common environment will become divided, one part joining the specific environment of Sx; the other part, the specific environment of S2, as shown in Fig. 8. In neither case will the species be in any way changed. This competition with the common environ- ment cannot cause the origin of a new species, but it may possibly lead to the extinction of one. Attention must here be drawn to the dif- ference between competition occurring within 54 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION a species, and competition between two or more species ; to the difference between intra- and inter-specific struggles. In intra-specific struggles, because the indi- viduals are very similar, the struggle will be prolonged : very small differences will decide I 4! I \ .1. ! 11 | E.g 1 1 "7" s., i As. FIG. 7. 1 1 1 \ II E.1 ! 1 I 1 ] i 1 T E a 1 I I I 5 1 E i .3 i FIG. 8. life and death. Under these conditions evolu- tion will proceed. In inter-specific struggles, which take place within common environments (when they be- come overcrowded) because the individuals are not similar in structure, the struggle will be soon over. One species is bound to be vastly more fit than the other, and will there- fore quickly displace it. Can a struggle be THE SELECTING POWER OF ENEMIES 55 long protracted between individuals as dif- ferent in structure as are two species? If it cannot, then such a struggle can have no effect as regards evolution, because a rela- tively long period of time is required both for the origin and fixation of variations. Two caterpillars, A and B, feed upon a certain tree ; the tree becomes rare ; competi- tion between A and B at once begins. In a few years A will displace and render B extinct, or vice versa ; because one must pos- sess certain characters, structures, and habits which give it a very decided advantage in the struggle ; or A will be able to turn out B, for instance, from the young leaves, and B turn out A from the old leaves ; A will be found feeding only on young leaves, B on old — there will be splitting of the common environment. In neither case can this competition in a common environment be a factor in evolution. Before proceeding further it is necessary to review the conclusions that have been made. 1. The boundaries of the environment of a species are definite. 2. The competition between species and species leads to either extinction of one or the splitting of the common environment into 56 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION two specific environments. Species generally occupy common environments without com- petition because there is no crowding there. Common environments do not give rise to specific structures. 3. New species are formed by differentiation of their specific environments. 4. The environment becomes similarly sexu- ally and socially differentiated. 5. Change in environment will produce either evolution or degeneration. If ontogeny and phylogeny give the history of a species, they should also give the history of its en- vironment. The earliest forms of life were probably all aquatic. The embryos of nearly all vertebrates and invertebrates have some characters and structures which indicate that their distant ancestors lived in water. The conceptions or confessions of faith embodied in most of the above arguments are of course not new ; they have been recalled because they lead the way to the subject under consideration, namely, the power of enemies as selecting agents. Particular attention is directed to con- clusions Nos. 2 and 3, which state that common environments do not give rise to specific structures, or in other words, the THE SELECTING POWER OF ENEMIES 57 selection of common environments is, re evolution and the origin of species, of much less importance than the selection of specific environments. Specific environments can often be recog- nised by their being usually restricted. The environment which restricts a species is a part of its specific environment. This will be found to be in the vast majority of cases either food supply or enemies. Food supply and enemies are closely connected ; if one is im- portant, both must be. A is the food supply of B : B is the enemy of A. The greatest disturbances which man has made in Nature have been due to alterations in one of these two factors. The two most efficient ways of either exterminating a species or controlling it are the destruction of its food supply or the increase of its enemies. In Nature's balance the weights are these two factors. Isolation is probably the greatest deter- mining factor in the preservation of species. It ensures protection from fresh enemies. Migration the greatest determining factor in the destruction of species. It introduces new enemies. Species with many enemies are the least variable. Protection from enemies causes species to vary, as in domesticated animals. 58 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION It is possible to arrive at the same con- clusion in other ways. There is evidence that the destruction of wild animals is in many respects similar to that observed in man : and that an animal life-table is very like a human one. Many are born to die almost at once : some die before reaching maturity ; others become adults ; and a few only reach old age. It has therefore been said that Natural Selection acts chiefly upon the young, and therefore must be most effective before the assumption of adult characters : and thus cannot be a powerful force in Evo- lution. But this is a false argument, apart from consideration of juvenile characters. Consider one thousand individuals, and sup- pose that ten of these become reproductively mature ; then it is obvious that the death of one of these ten will be equivalent to the destruction of one hundred newly born in- dividuals ; which means that the power of Natural Selection, re Evolution, when adults are operated upon, is a hundred times as great as when newly born individuals are dealt with, and still greater when the adults are about to reproduce. It follows, therefore, that the factors which decide life or death of adults are very important ones of Natural Selection. THE SELECTING POWER OF ENEMIES 59 Evidence will now be brought forward that the most important of the selecting factors, the one always at work, is the species' enemies. Consider temperature ; a severe winter kills off those with poor resisting power to cold ; then summer's drought comes, and another batch is destroyed. The following winter is mild, so the next generation are not subjected to selection by cold. A very hot summer destroys many of these, perhaps just those which are able to best resist cold : so that any advance in ability to resist cold, as a result of the first selection, will be lost, on account of the inconstancy of the selecting agent and the swamping effects of the other agents. But with enemies, the case is different ; they are always present, destroying through all the year ; and their action will be accumulative. There is a property of enemies that requires to be especially mentioned. They destroy suddenly. There is no time for the species to protect itself by means of acquired char- acters. This power which enables organic matter to adapt itself to diversities of en- vironment is valueless in respect of enemies : only hereditary characters can be used for this purpose. Therefore, as hereditary characters 60 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION usually form specific characters and acquired characters do not, it follows that the char- acters to do with protection from enemies are likely to be very important ones. Much criticism has been levelled against natural selec- tion from a mathematical point of view, and especially as regards the swamping effects of chance. If chance kills off 950 of a 1000 individuals, then natural selection has 50 instead of 1000 to work with. However, the effect must be the same, except that longer time will be re- quired for the origin of favourable variations. When, however, after perhaps many generations the favourable variation does arise and escape the hands of chance, then it will be one amongst 50 instead of one in 1000, and therefore less likely to be swamped. False examples of chance are often cited. The stepping of a horse on an ant's nest is a typical one. The action of the horse may be the result of chance. But what can be said about the position of the nest ? The mother ant in the past chose this particular spot for her nest. Other mothers chose in some cases no doubt places where horses are not likely to walk. Will not such mothers therefore be thus selected to carry on the race? In conclusion it may be said that, although the subject has been approached from several standpoints, reasoning has led in each case to the same two conclusions : 1. Specific environments are more impor- tant than common environments as selecting agents. 2. Species' enemies are very important THE SELECTING POWER OF ENEMIES 61 factors of specific environments : and must be powerful selecting agents calling forth important corresponding structures. One other conclusion that has been arrived at may here be mentioned. As has been truly pointed out, the welfare of a species is almost of as much importance to the species' enemy as to the species itself. In spite of this, some naturalists expect to find highly protectively coloured animals almost free from enemies. Were this so, then there would be no selection, there would be nothing to maintain this specialisation, which according to present-day arguments would at once begin to degenerate until its efficacy failed, permitting the enemy to again destroy. It seems, therefore, that the most efficient protective coloration can only be maintained by a severe selection by enemies. So only can be produced and maintained other highly specialised characters such as warning coloration. One must therefore expect to observe that animals which present a high degree of protective or warning coloration, frequently form food for their predaceous enemies. CHAPTER V FAMILY INSTINCTS IT may appear strange that instincts are first chosen to illustrate this Theory, for of all characters they are the least known or under- stood, and only in recent years have they been systematically studied, and been given an im- portant place among characters as a whole. They have been chosen because, as family characters, they well and clearly illustrate the Theory. The vast majority of parents have an in- stinct which compels them to sacrifice them- selves for the sake of their young ; this sacrificing instinct is present in both sexes, and varies according to the age of the young. When the progeny is very young it is strongest, and as it grows older so the instinct becomes less strong : finally, when the progeny is full grown, it entirely ceases, and in many cases is replaced by another instinct, which compels the parents to drive away their progeny. The FAMILY INSTINCTS 63 possession of this parental instinct has an effect on the selection of enemies within the family : the likelihood of their selecting young is much reduced. As has been shown, young are more valuable to the species than old; therefore the species controls Natural Selec- tion in such a way, that more valuable young are preserved at the expense of less valuable parents. Families having this character are more likely to bring their young to maturity, and to thus reproduce themselves, than families in which this parental character is faint or absent. Thus, the FAMILY comes to possess this character which has arisen as a variation and has been preserved through heredity : just as within the individual, particular forms, struc- tures, or colours or other character having been born, becomes fixed. Natural Selection, dealing with families, produces family characters whose function is to preserve the more valuable members of the family, at the expense of the less valuable. To return to parental instincts, closely allied to the sacrificing instinct is a combative one. Parents will attack an enemy of their young. If they prove victorious, the instinct which directed their actions may be called combative ; 64 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION if they are beaten in the fight, it will be called sacrificing. There is another parental instinct, commonly found in birds, which may be called an attract- ing instinct. It consists of unusual and con- spicuous movements whose object is to distract an enemy from their young and entice it to attack the parents ; these are often described as feints because the birds appear to pretend to have broken legs or wings, or to be other- wise wounded and crippled. By these attract- ing instincts the parents draw an enemy's attack, and having accomplished this, then make good their own escape. During these feints, several ways of making themselves conspicuous are utilised : bright colours are displayed ; wings, wherein are bright bars, are opened ; tails containing white feathers are spread ; breasts or backs are turned, according as they are brightly coloured, towards the enemy. Strange forms are assumed ; combs are erected, tails spread, wings opened and necks thrown out ; and conspicuous sounds are made. By these and other methods, parents make themselves conspicuous. When pre-copulatory displays are considered, it will be shown what a close similarity there is between these parental displays, these attracting instincts, and the dis- FAMILY INSTINCTS 65 plays made before copulation, which are usually called the displays of courtship. It will be further shown that, whereas the parental displays are to protect the young, the sexual displays of the male are to protect the female. Whilst the parents are responding to these instincts, the young are behaving quite dif- ferently. Eeacting to a warning of danger, given (either vocally or by other ways) to them by the parents, they instinctively make themselves as inconspicuous as possible : they at once squat or seek cover. This family character possessed by the young must con- trol Natural Selection within the family, just as the parental instincts do. The young of a family who instinctively seek cover, when warned by the parents, have a better chance of surviving and of reproducing themselves, than the young who do not thus respond to their parents' call. In like manner can be accounted for very many of the instincts found confined to parents, and those possessed solely by the young. Further, as will be shown later, many of the characters found confined to either parent or young, no matter what their nature, either structure, form, or colour, can be similarly explained. Instincts have been chosen first for con- E 66 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION sideration because they are self-evidently a means whereby Natural Selection within a family is controlled. Many account for the parental sacrificing instinct by concluding that the young and the juvenile, because they are helpless and inexperienced in defence, require protection of their parents. But such an argument is not sound. They require protection because they are valuable to the species : their help- lessness adds nothing to their value. How- ever, apart from this question : the origin of these characters as variations confined to particular members of a society and their fixation, must have been through Natural Selection dealing with families, and not with individuals ; and further, these characters must control Natural Selection in such a way that one set of individuals, the young, is rendered more liable to be selected for sur- vival than the other, the parents. CHAPTER VI THE COPULATORY ATTITUDES OF THE PAIR ANIMALS form themselves into a number of different societies. In the last chapter, the society of a family was considered in respect of one of its characters, its instincts ; another society has now to occupy the mind — the pair, the associated male and female. There is probably no society in Nature more close than the copulating pair. And because the pair thus united are especially open to at- tack, Natural Selection will probably leave its mark on these two individuals. As has been clearly proved, females are more valuable than males ; search should therefore reveal the presence of characters in the pair, which control Natural Selection in such a way that during copulation females will be preserved at the expense of males. At present, only one character of the copulating pair will be dealt with — the at- titudes they assume during copulation. A 67 68 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION hasty consideration might lead to the expres- sion of opinion that copulatory attitudes depended on the position, structure, and mechanism of the copulatory organs : but the facts of Nature lead one to argue that she does not mould organs and then leave the individual to fit them as best he can ; instead, one is encouraged to believe that organs are made for use in a particular way. The ques- tion that is now asked is — Do the copula- tory attitudes assumed by animals, in any way assist in the preservation of the female at the expense of the male ? In mammals and birds, the position of the male above and behind the female must un- doubtedly render him, in the event of an attack by an enemy, far more liable to be killed. A couple are especially liable to be successfully attacked from behind ; a frontal attack would be in their full view and likely to fail. A hawk stooping at a pair of birds, must strike the male. In mammals, the male, standing on his hind legs, is helpless compared to the female, who, standing on all four, is able to spring aside on the instant. Further, experience has taught predatory animals that it is unwise to leave a stricken victim until it is certainly dead; for this reason, seldom THE COPULATORY ATTITUDES 69 more than one of a pair is likely to be killed in an attack. The male's death is nothing to the species, for he probably has already ferti- lised the female, if not during the last copula- tion, at a former one ; her death would be a great loss to the species, the loss of very valuable fertilised ova. The length of time taken by the copula- tory act varies with the species, and among mammals is longest in the predatory ones ; compare, in this respect, the Felidae Canidae and Mustelidae with the Suidae, Cervidae Equidae or Muridae and Leporidae. Among birds, I am aware of only one ex- ception to the rule that the male mounts the hen — Swans (Cygnus olor), which copulate breast to breast, and so will apparently be equally liable to be killed if then attacked. It is noteworthy, that of all birds, Swans are probably the least liable to attack, because of their pugnacity and great strength. In the majority of insects, the copulatory period is often prolonged for many hours or even days ; during this long period the pairs are in a helpless condition, able only to make feeble attempts to escape from danger. In many species, the male rests on top of the female, as it were guarding her and often 70 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION carrying her in flight. In other species, the pairs unite end to end ; in these cases, other methods encouraging the taking of the male are utilised, as will be shown later. But although it seems that when thus united, an enemy would chance to take male and female equally, in many cases, owing to the position in space occupied by the copulating pair, this is not so ; for instance, in some cases the male clings to a grass stem or other support, whilst the female dangles below ; a bird taking such a pair will strike first where the insect joins the grass, having learnt by experience that of insects, this, the head end, is the most vulnerable part ; the bird will seize the male, the female becoming unjoined will be likely to escape. In such ways as this, by the assumption of a particular position on the part of the pair, will the taking of males rather than females be encouraged. « When copulating insects are given to birds, it has been observed that when one has been seized, the other is apparently mistaken by the bird for some foreign object, which the bird endeavours to and does get rid of by rapidly shaking its head : those insects which are firmly joined together and only with difficulty parted will thus be disconnected. The copulating habits of the Common THE COPULATORY ATTITUDES 71 Sparrow (Passer domesticus) are worthy of mention. Whilst the successful male is per- forming the act, the pair are usually closely surrounded by a number of chattering males. If this company of birds be attacked, it is not likely that the female will be taken, but rather one of the many males. I have twice seen a Hawk take a Sparrow under these conditions, and on each occasion it was a male. These two observations of course prove nothing, but, at the same time, it appeared to me to be impossible for the hawk to chance to take the female ; a chance greater than the relative number of males to females. So with certain insects, in which, during copulation, the pairs are surrounded by a host of males, these must protect the female, whether this be the aim of such a gathering or no. The above applies to many Diptera, the Bees (Apiarice), Termidae, and others. Lastly, in bisexual or hermaphrodite animals, there is no difference in the relative position of the two individuals as would be expected according to the Theory, for they are of the same value. During copulation, they always make a symmetrical figure. Thus in conclusion it may, I think, be fairly stated that the position assumed by the pair 72 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION during copulation renders males more liable to destruction than females. In this way the species may be said to control Natural Selec- tion, so that valueless males may be destroyed rather than valuable females. CHAPTER VII PRE-COPTJLATORY DISPLAYS BEFORE copulation, the male commonly makes some display; such are often called the dis- plays of courtship, implying an association with mating ; but as many animals mate only once in a lifetime or in a season, whilst these displays are made before each copulation, they are better described by being called Pre- copulatory Displays. A study of them shows that they undoubtedly render the male con- spicuous and purposely so. All the methods mentioned in Chapters II and III are em- ployed : movements are made, forms assumed, attitudes struck, sounds produced, scents secreted and vivid colours are laid bare, all of which make the male conspicuous in Nature. The female, during this display of the male, remains silent and motionless, and usually more or less concealed. Here, then, is material to which the Theory can be applied : Two individuals, male and 73 74 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION female, unequal in value, associated together, and at a time when the difference in their values is greatest, i.e. during the breeding season ; the male, the less valuable, renders himself conspicuous in many ways ; the female keeps more or less concealed. Here are two associated individuals of different value, pre- senting different characters. According to the Theory, these characters, or at least some of them, should control Natural Selection in such a way that the less valuable male will be taken, rather than the more valuable female. Further, at this time, the pair are especially liable to be successfully attacked, because their attention is fully occupied with sexual affairs ; so that some provision for the selec- tion of the male may be expected. Therefore may it not be, that the male renders himself conspicuous in order that, in the event of the pair being attacked by an enemy, he may be destroyed rather than the female ? The Hawk will stoop, the Cat will spring, at the more easily seen bird. It does not follow that one of the two must in every case perish ; the display of the male will draw the attack of any neighbouring enemy and thus, as it were, clear the air, so that dangerous copulation may be accomplished in safety. PRE-COPULATORY DISPLAYS 75 This, in fact, is the meaning of such display, its purpose. Direct evidence of this is, and possibly must always be, wanting. The period of destruction of an animal forms so minute a portion of its whole life, that a single person, even if constantly observ- ing, could record only a few deaths, and of these only a small percentage would be deaths of either male or female during sexual display. Experiments could be arranged to test the theory. Pairs of animals could be subjected to their enemies. Although direct evidence is wanting, some indirect evidence appears to give support. To the vast majority of animals man is an enemy. His presence causes them to stop doing the affair of the day, and to seek cover. But his presence often does not cause the cessation of pre-copulatory displays ; on the other hand it may cause them to be more vigorously performed. In fact his presence calls forth this male reaction as powerfully as the presence of the female. Numerous examples have been recorded of display in the absence of the female, and when only man or an enemy is at hand. 76 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION Another favouring fact is that animals, not liable to be attacked, either make no pre- copulatory displays or only poor ones. It is also noteworthy that the displays made by the parents for the protection of their young during the attack of an enemy, are very similar to and sometimes exactly like pre- copulatory displays. In one case an enemy is the stimulating agent ; may it not be so in both? Just as there is no direct evidence in favour of this conception, so there is none against it. Other theories have been propounded to account for the facts ; these will be discussed under Sexual Colour (Chapter VIII) ; and what contrary indirect evidence there is will be also then considered. Some have likened these displays to car- esses, and have supposed that by them the male overcomes a coyness of the female. Females appear to be coy be- cause they take no notice of the male's display; for this reason, when the male's display appears to be kindly in nature to- wards the female, it is called a caress ; and no heed is taken of the many cases in which the male's display appears quite brutal. Further, much coyness on the part of the PRE-COPULATORY DISPLAYS 77 female must, one would think, be harmful to the species. This explanation therefore appears unsatis- factory, even for the small number of displays which it could account for. CHAPTEE VIII SEXUAL COLOUR MALES and females are frequently coloured differently. With comparatively few excep- tions, the males are more brightly coloured, especially during the breeding season. These characters are widely distributed throughout the animal kingdom, and this universality indicates that the cause must be of a world- wide nature. In some cases it has been observed that the sexes occupy either entirely, or somewhat different, environments, hence their colour differences. But in other cases, environmental differ- ences, corresponding to these colour and other structural differences, have not been found. For this reason other theories have been propounded to account for them. Darwin's sexual selection is the most gener- ally accepted theory. This states that females select males which have characters pleasing to them, bright colours for instance. Assume 78 SEXUAL COLOUR 79 that this is so and that males in this way become altered in character. Such a male character must be either beneficial or detri- mental to the species as a whole ; it cannot be neutral. If detrimental, the species must either become extinct, or Natural Selection will remove the offending character ; if bene- ficial, then Natural Selection would have fixed the male's useful variation without the assistance of female selection. Characters, confined to the male, can be fixed by Natural Selection alone, and thus selection on the part of the female is not necessary to explain their origin. In order that females, by selec- tion, may alter the characters of males, two monosexual variations (one in the male, the other in the female) must arise simultaneously, and further, these must be related. For in- stance, a bright colour must arise as a varia- tion in the male, and at the same time there must be born in the female as a variation, an instinct which compels her to prefer this bright-coloured male. To conceive of this is difficult. On the other hand, Natural Selection would require only one variation to arise. If a beneficial character arises, it will become fixed, although the variation be confined to 80 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION one sex; this conception is easier. Such a variation would be correlated with the sexual determinant ; variations not so correlated or bound to a sexual determinant cannot, one would conceive, be made to do so by Natural Selection : for instance, favour their fixation in the male, and their eradication in the female. Sexual Selection, if it explains the origin of extra sexual characters, does not explain their use, yet, as has just been shown, they must be useful, or otherwise Natural Selec- tion would remove them. Beside this argument, there are many facts that sexual selection fails to correlate and which it ought to correlate. They mostly come under the following headings : 1. Sex selection accounts for the brilliant colours of males, but fails to account for equally brilliant colours which are frequently found in both sexes. 2. Sexual selection, if it has been observed, has not been observed to be especially asso- ciated with animals which show the greatest sexual differences in characters. Birds, for instance, in which the sexes are similar, do not show less selection by the female of the male, than birds whose males are of SEXUAL COLOUR 81 the most brilliant colour compared to their females. 3. If females select then, there must be either more males than females or polygamy : but in many insects which present marked sexual differences, males and females are equal in number. Further, very few examples of selection by females have been recorded ; whereas many experiments have been carried out that conclusively show that females do not select. 4. In many animals, the male undoubtedly selects the female, and yet the male is the more brightly coloured. 5. The displays of courtship are not to do with mating, but, as has been shown, are pre-copulatory displays ; they therefore do not play the r61e in sexual selection which is assigned to them. 6. Further, these displays are not made only by animals which present secondary sexual differences in characters, but are also made by animals whose sexes are similar. 7. Sexual selection does not explain the absence of conspicuous male colouring in predatory and night animals ; or rather they form unexplained exceptions. Of course there must be many facts which F 82 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION the theory of sexual selection explains or correlates, else it could never have been con- ceived. Nevertheless much has been written against sexual selection, especially in Germany and America, and a general demand made for another explanation.1 There are other theories. Wallace explains the dull dresses of female birds and insects by supposing that the young- producing females require better protection than the males. But one would think that if dull plumes are an advantage to the female they would also be of use to the male, and that therefore he would don them too. This theory of Wallace apparently comes very near to the one under description, but in reality it is almost the converse. The new theory states that the male becomes brilliant in colour in order that he may be more likely to be destroyed : and thus the dull-coloured female gain protection. In Wallace's theory the female becomes altered for her sake, in this theory the male for the female's sake. Wallace 1 The author's theory is not, strictly speaking, antagonistic to Sexual Selection, but only in so far that such selection cannot be for the purpose of gratifying the female's sense of beauty. The new theory is quite able to embrace female selection provided they instinctively select males which will be more attractive to enemies than males in general ; for instance, con- spicuously coloured males as against beautiful males. SEXUAL COLOUR 83 explains the dull colour of females but not the brilliant colour of males. Emery's theory of the origin of secondary sexual characters as mono-sexual sports may explain their origin, although there appears to be little evidence of this, but makes no attempt to define the cause of these differ- ences ; or assumes that the sport is retained because and when it fits the environment better than the old type. Those who believe in the transmission of ac- quired characters look upon secondary sexual characters as adaptations and due to the stimulation of parts through use. Controlled natural selection accounts for both the origin and purpose of secondary sexual characters in the following way. Males are more con- spicuous in nature than females : males are less valuable than females. Males and females are associated together during life, and especi- ally during the breeding season when the difference in colour is greatest, and when their difference in value is highest : therefore accord- ing to the theory, the conspicuous colour of the male serves to control natural selection in such a way that the less valuable male will be killed in preference to the more valuable female. 84 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION The occurrence of only a single variation is necessary for this conception. Because females who happen to choose the most conspicuous males, will be more liable to escape destruc- tion than females who happen to be mated with less conspicuous males : and the former females will therefore be more likely to hand on this male character than the latter females. This applies to all the purposely conspicuous characters of males. Stolzmann in 1885 put forward a theory to account for the secondary sexual characters of birds, which in some respects resembles this theory. Having observed that males are often more numerous than females, he justly concluded that the species' food supply was being wasted on them and that this excess of males was harmful to the species ; more especially be- cause he believed that the paired females during the breeding season were harried by celibate males, with injurious results. He therefore conceived that the brilliant colours of the males arose in order that they might easily be seen by enemies and their ranks thus thinned. This theory was very short-lived, because it was speedily pointed out, that if males SEXUAL COLOUR 85 become conspicuous in order to be killed, only less conspicuous ones will be left to carry on the species, and conspicuousness will thus be quickly eradicated by Natural Selection. It is obvious that this criticism cannot be levelled at the author's theory, which in some respects is not unlike Stolzmann's. The author's theory deals with the pair, male and female associated together at the breeding season or during copulation ; under these con- ditions the destruction of a conspicuous male by no means precludes that his characters will not be handed down to the next generation. He most probably will have already fertilised the female either partially or entirely. It may here be noted that secondary sexual characters are at their highest development at the time when the female is ready to be fertilised ; male birds, for instance, are in their most brilliant plumage, not during their nest- ing season, but at a considerable time before this, in either the last of winter or the first of spring. The relative proportion of males to females is, as Stolzmann concludes, a most important factor in the welfare of the species. Accord- ing to the theory, great excess of males 86 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION should be associated with great secondary sexual difference, and vice versa. In some animals, an excess of males is pro- duced by special methods, e.g. ants and bees. It appears not so much to ensure the fertili- sation of all females, as to render the chance of their destruction small. It is noteworthy that polygamy is usually associated with great secondary sexual differences ; these latter, ac- cording to the theory, give rise to great destruction of males, hence the association, as in the peacock. In predatory animals, polygamy is very rare ; as are also great secondary sexual differences. If, as some maintain, the purpose of eye-spots in insects is to entice enemies to strike them, rather than more vulnerable parts : may not the eye-spots on the peacock's (Pavo Cristatus) tail be to entice an enemy away from the female, to draw its attack? When considering the parental colouring of birds, it will be noted that confinement of conspicuous colouring to the male is asso- ciated with open-nesting habits. It is possible that, among birds, conspicuous colouring con- fined to the male arose in this way (Wallace), and that afterwards it has become greatly evoluted by Natural Selection working with pairs. There is some evidence that open- SEXUAL COLOUR 87 nesting cannot alone account for such great differentiation, for when males incubate, females are never more than slightly more conspicuously coloured. It appears, there- fore, that the difference in value between the sexes, or some other factor, must be looked for to explain this. As with parental colour, so with sexual colour — conspicuous colours among preda- tory animals, or among night animals, would not be anticipated, nor is it found. So also sexual dimorphism should, accord- ing to the theory, not be found in aposematic animals, nor is it at all common ; because, having few enemies, they are not very liable to attack. Sometimes secondary sexual char- acters occur before sexual maturity, as in the upland Goose (Chloephaga Magellanica). In this case one would conclude that the society feels the great value of the female even at this early stage. Worthy of note and favouring the theory is the fact that in mimicry it is often only the female which seeks protection in this way; the male, it would seem, purposely refraining from doing so, in order that, should the pair be attacked during mating or copulation, he will be destroyed at the saving of the female. 88 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION Finally, it will be well to classify secondary sexual characters in the light of this new theory. MONO-SEXUAL CHARACTERS : 1. Characters that are adaptations to a mono- sexual environment ; for instance, associated with a food supply confined to one sex, with copulation, with the rearing of young, with the recognition or finding of one sex by the other, &c. 2. Characters whose purpose is to control Natural Selection within the pair, value marks; for instance, attracting colour, sound, scent, &c. confined to the male. It must be clearly understood that the second cause of mono-sexual characters is, like the first, an environmental character. It is the result of selection by enemies, which selec- tion has been shown to be one of the most important factors of a species' specific en- vironment. For this reason it should not, theoretically, be given a major heading in the classification of the causes of mono-sexual characters. There is, however, a side issue which to some extent justifies this arrange- ment ; for the species behaves towards enemies in a particular manner. It adjusts this selection by enemies so that, SEXUAL COLOUR 89 when the members of a species are not all of equal value, the less valuable will be more likely to be destroyed than the more valuable. It is as if the species, realising that it must form the food supply of another species and that certain of its members must be destroyed, conserved its valuable members, by casting its valueless ones to satisfy the enemy. This it does, not actively but passively, by so clothing the valueless that they will be more easily seen by the enemy and thus more liable to be attacked. As before mentioned, it does not follow that in every case they will be destroyed, they may make good their escape ; but by drawing the attack, the valuable will be preserved from attack. Further, it is obvious that there will be a limit to this value marking : as, for instance, when males become less numerous than females, so their relative value will increase ; they are not entirely valueless. When they thus become of equal value as the female, further alteration in structure will cease. The limit to which this differentiation will proceed, will depend on the number of females a single male is able to fertilise, or his initial 90 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION value to the species in other respects. The greater the polygamy the greater the sexual difference, should be the rule. This, I think, most will agree, is borne out in Nature. Thus it appears that sexual differentiation is fre- quently an expression of selection by enemies. CHAPTER IX PARENTAL COLOUR A COMPARISON of the colour of parents and young shows, that whereas young are almost invariably protectively coloured, parents are not uncommonly conspicuously coloured. This difference in colour is explained by some, who say that the protective colouring of young animals is the colour of their ancestors, which they must assume ^during development, be- cause development reproduces the ancestry of the species : and who say that protective colouring must therefore be assumed, whether beneficial or not : and who look upon the conspicuous colouring of many adults as a more recent added character. There is no doubt that an animal, during development, does reproduce its ancestry, but then there is no evidence that it must do so. Not all the characters of its ancestors are retained, but only a few : and of the few, many do not attain to as high a development as 92 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION was present in the ancestors. Is it not reason- able to suppose that only those characters are retained which are useful, or that of a given character only so much as is useful : or else why are so many completely lost ? The Tadpole (Rana temporaries) has first external gills, then internal gills, and finally lungs, all of which are put to use : so too it is possible that the gill clefts or their rudi- ments found in the human embryo then serve some useful purpose. Natural Selection must deal with ova, embryos, juveniles, as it does with adults, and produce embryonic or juvenile characters. Ova and embryos, like adults, have environments to which they must adapt themselves. We are accustomed to 'study chiefly adult characters and to forget that adult is but a stage, merging on the one side into youth, and on the other, old age ; each of which have important characters. Some of the characters of old age may be looked upon as means by which the species rids itself of an individual which has shed all its germ cells. Selection must occur with reference to a character, at the time of, or after, the appear- ance of the character, and not before. Thus the characters that go to form an embryo or PARENTAL COLOUR 93 young animal, or an adult, are of two kinds — (1) structures, or parts of structures, which their ancestors possessed, and which have been retained because they are useful ; (2) added structures, which their distant ancestors never possessed, to fit some new environment ; these may be added at any stage of develop- ment from the egg to the complete adult. Therefore the hereditary explanation of the difference of colour between young and adult is not sufficient. That this is so is also proved by the whole of experimental embryology, and also by the fact that examples can be found of young animals not presenting protective colouring, and almost certainly not having the colour of their ancestors. One must conclude, therefore, that young animals are as a rule protectively coloured, and adults often con- spicuously coloured, for a purpose. An examination of the species which present protectively coloured young and conspicuously coloured adults, reveals the fact that in the vast majority of cases young and parents are associated together during a portion of their lives ; and further, that it is whilst thus associated that the difference is most marked — e.g. parents are most brilliant during the breeding season. Examples of conspicuously 94 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION coloured parents and protectively coloured young are to be found widely distributed in the animal kingdom. In fact, it is exceptional to find parents, bringing up young, which do not show this difference in colour or other character. Thus are brought together the three factors with which Controlled Natural Selection can work : (1) A society of animals (2) Of different value, and (3) Having different characters. During Natural Selection the less valuable and conspicuous parents will be taken, at the saving of the more valuable and inconspicuous young. The predatory animal will take the conspicuous parents that it can best see, and leave the young that are only seen with difficulty. When dealing with Parental Instincts, men- tion was made of the displays of colour that are made by the parent when the family is attacked; they will show their brightest plumes to the enemy and endeavour to draw its attack ; they will display bright colours which are otherwise concealed in wings and tail (see Concealed Conspicuous Colours, Chapter III). Predatory animals, because they have no enemies, should not present conspicuous colour- PARENTAL COLOUR 95 ing, neither do they ; they are almost always protectively coloured. For instance, of the Falconidae, only the small insectivorous species that are liable to be attacked by the more powerful kinds show conspicuous colouring. For similar reasons, no night birds should present, nor do they, conspicuous colouring. There are, however, many birds which present conspicuous colour- ing only in the male : with very few exceptions these are found to make open nests, which require the female to be protectively coloured ; for this reason, only the male is conspicuously coloured (when males incubate, females are the more conspicuous). There still remain many which are neither predatory nor night birds, and which, nevertheless, are protectively coloured in both sexes ; it is among these birds that conspicuous displays are seen best, and most marked. The position these birds will take up, the motion they will make in an endeavour thus to protect their young, are remarkable — the Meadow-pipit (Anthus pra- tensis), the Reed-bunting (Emberiza schceni- clus), and the Partridge (Perdix cinerea) are well-known examples. Only by the collection of a large number of facts such as these can the theory become a law. As before mentioned, 96 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION it is now only being introduced as a working hypothesis. It will probably not explain all cases of conspicuous colouring among parents, for it has been shown that conspicuous colour- ing may be used for other purposes. Attraction marks (see page 40, Chapter III) may be especially mentioned, for although they are conspicuous they are found in both parents and young. The rule appears to be : (1) vital parts that are conspicuously coloured in parents are protectively coloured in young, and (2) non-vital parts conspicuously coloured in parents are similarly coloured in young. A few examples are necessary: (1) The Goldfinch (Carduelis elegans) : yellow bar across the wing, found in both parents and young; other conspicuous colours on vital parts of parents are not found in young, which are protectively coloured ; (2) Wheat- ear (Saxicola cenanthe) : white rump present in both parents and young ; (3) Wood-pigeon (Columba palumbus) : white patch on side of neck in parent, and because on a vital part, not present in young; (4) Mallard (Anasbascus): speculum on wing found in young and old, in all plumages. It appears, therefore, that some of the differ- ences in colour between parents and young, PARENTAL COLOUR 97 like the differences between male and female, may be due to the selection of enemies work- ing on individuals of different value ; may in fact be on one hand value marks, and on the other adaptation to enemies. CHAPTER X PARENTAL AND SEXUAL CHARACTERS MENTION has been made that among birds, for instance, there are many which do not present conspicuous colouring, in either sex. It has also been shown (under parental instincts) how these birds protect their young by making themselves conspicuous other than by means of colour ; similarly it has been noted that female protection is accomplished in these birds by pre-copu- latory displays on the part of the male, quite apart from colour. In these crypti- cally coloured birds, another male character is also possibly used for female protection, namely — Song. The production of sounds during pre- copulatory displays are obviously a part of the display. But the songs of birds cannot be thus classed. Singing birds are conspicuous in nature, PARENTAL AND SEXUAL CHARACTERS 99 and there is considerable evidence that they always sing for this purpose. When perform- ing, the males invariably take up prominent positions, the tops of trees, high in the air, on the summits of rocks, &c., whence they may easily be seen from all around. Further, many of them sing when disturbed or frightened by an enemy ; a stone cast into a reed-bed or thicket will at once start the warblers in full chorus. They sing in the morning and evening when predatory animals are feeding. They sing when a hawk or weasel shows itself in the copse. They do not sing to their females, who may be out of hearing, and who take no notice of the song, but proceed with the affairs of the day, unaffected. May not this male character be brought into the line which many other male characters have been considered to take, namely, that its purpose is to attract possible enemies, so that comparatively value- less males may perish, rather than their valu- able females, or at least draw the enemy's attack ? The instrumentation of the males of insects may also be, in many cases, for this purpose. Sound -producing males are most commonly found in palatable insects. Scent may similarly be used for female 100 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION protection ; the absence of scent in many hen birds which nest on the ground, in- dicates that scent plays a greater part in Nature than our feeble sense organ would lead us to imagine. The pleasant perfume which some male insects have, may be to draw an enemy's attention to them, during copulation, rather than to the female. In offensively scented insects, the sexes are usually similar, and the character is probably of a warning nature. This concludes the majority of the char- acters of conspicuousness which distinguish parents from young, male from female : in the one case, young as protected at the ex- pense of parents ; in the other, females at the expense of males. The following table indicates how precisely similar are the methods used ; examples are given from among birds : PARENTAL AND SEXUAL CH£.ftA'CTERS TABLE I PROTECTION OF YOUNG BY PARENTS Position. Form. Movement. Sound. Scent. Colour. display PAEENTS take up promi- nent positions : erect plumes, spread tails and wings. simulate injury, flutter, &c. make various noises — the cack- ling hen. (no ex- amples found.) their bright plumes and have bright colours. remain iTOUNG lie low, squat. conceal their form. quite motion- less or remain silent. (no ex- amples found.) are cryp- tically coloured. retreat. PROTECTION OF FEMALE BY MALE does not M 1 pre-copu- latory attitudes. pre-copu- latory erection of plumes, many curious pre-copu- latory move- pre-copu- latory sounds, also lose scent : in many insects has at- tracting colour. &c. songs. sweet- scented. FEMALE remains for the most part con- cealed. hides form. quiescent. quiet. scentless during breeding season. is crypti- cally coloured. If, as some will be willing to admit, the parents use these various ways for the pro- 102 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION tection of their young (instinctively), why should not similar performances and exhibi- tions on the part of the male, be to protect the female ? A more detailed comparison of the means the parent uses to protect the young, with the pre-copulatory displays of the male, shows how strikingly similar are not only the methods used, but the actual move- ments, positions, and sounds, &c., made. In one case a given stimulus, the presence of an enemy coupled with possession of young, causes the parent to instinctively react ; in the other a given stimulus, the presence of an enemy, either real or assumed, coupled with the sexual possession of a female, causes the male to react in a similar manner. And just as attracting instincts in both sexes serve to protect the young, and in the male to protect the female, so the attracting colour of parents and males will have the same effect. Further, these displays, pre-copulatory and for young protection, are often made when neither females nor young are present, but at the approach of an enemy; they are then called expressions of fear, instead of reactions to a stimulus. May not this be the stimulus always, no matter when they are displayed ? PARENTAL AND SEXUAL CHARACTERS 103 For each species a table can be drawn up, like the above table of generalities ; for in- stance, in the case of a blackbird (Turdus merula) the following table results : TABLE II PROTECTION OF YOUNG BY PARENTS Position. Form. Movement. Sound. Scent. Colour. PARENTS show them- selves promi- nently in the hedge row. droop wings, spread tails. hop and flutter from branch to branch. loud cackling noises. nil. male attract- ing colour. YOUNG remain con- cealed. remain con- cealed. remain still. silent. nil. young crypti- cally coloured. PROTECTION OF FEMALE BY MALE • 3 H takes up promi- nent positions on trees, &c. droops wings, and spreads tail. hops and flutters from branch to branch. incessant, loud cackling, and song. nil. has at- tracting colour (black). FEMALE does not do so. does not alter form. remains still. silent. nil. crypti- cally coloured. 104 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION To this must be added the sacrificing and combative instincts which are used by the parents for the protection of the young, and by the male for the protection of the female. Female protection by this method is not very common in Nature ; the best examples are to be found in the higher animals, and especi- ally those which live together in societies : herds are often thus protected by the males. All possible combinations, of attracting and protecting colorations, will now be briefly considered. (7= conspicuous or attracting coloration. /= inconspicuous or cryptically coloured. P= parents. Y= young. P C Very common, especially in species Y 1 which form themselves in societies of families ; but also rarely in other species in which the parents do not look after the young. To protect the more valu- able young, in the latter case, in the absence of the formation of a society. P I Almost unknown, and forming ex- Y C ceptions to the rule. Examples can often be explained; for instance, the young frog, the tadpole, when black and conspicuous, is not palatable. PARENTAL AND SEXUAL CHARACTERS 105 PC By no means uncommon ; in the vast Y C majority of these cases the conspicuous characters of the young are like those of the parents, and, like them, are of a warning nature. P I A common condition when the species Y I does not form itself into the society or family ; but when families are found, some conspicuous character is almost always found in the parents, which is not present in the young. $ C Is very common in all mono-sexual £ / animals : to protect the more valuable female. $ I Very rare and form exceptions to the 4 C theory ; can sometimes be explained by their having unusual habits, as in the Phalaropes ; or by their being pro- tectively coloured to different environ- ments, although not at first sight obviously so. $ C Not uncommon, and almost always 4 C the conspicuous characters are warning. $ I Not uncommon ; colour or form may 4 / be similar in the two sexes and pro- tective, but they are seldom entirely 106 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION alike ; usually the male will be found to be conspicuous in one or more char- acters, as in song birds. In conclusion it may be said that many, if not the majority of those characters which distinguish males from females, are similar to the differences found between parents and young. Males equal females plus some other char- acters, females equal young plus some char- acters. In each case the added characters are alike, and according to the theory serve the same purpose. CHAPTER XI CONSIDERATION OF BRITISH BIRDS IN order to demonstrate, in still more detail, the applicability of the theory, it will now be applied to the colour of adult British Birds during the breeding season. This material is chosen because it is better to treat a small amount of material exhaustively, than to pick out from a large quantity (aves) only favour- able examples ; and because the habits and colours of British Birds are very well and widely known : also because birds, as a whole, are thus by no means badly exemplified ; and birds well exemplify the theory. They are classified in the following way, and will be considered accordingly : II. Birds of prey. 2. Night birds. 3. Song birds. 4. Other birds. 2. Female attracting ( 5. Open nests. Male attracting \ 6. Closed nests. 3. Female cryptic ( 7. Open nests. Male attracting \ 8. Closed nests. 4. Female attracting f 9. Open nests (2 birds). Male cryptic \ 10. Closed nests (nil). 107 108 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION The following method of placing the birds in the tables is employed. Birds presenting defined, unconcealed, flat areas of red, blue, yellow, black or white are placed under "at- tracting " ; the rest under " cryptic." In a few cases difficulties arise ; for instance, the Ring Dove (C. palumbus) has a small white patch on either side of the neck, and should therefore have been placed under "attract- ing " : but because the area of white is, com- pared to the bird, very small, and the bird otherwise undoubtedly cryptically coloured, it has been placed under " cryptic." DIVISION I Birds of Prey. — See Table I. Without exception, none of these birds present conspicuous colouring because they are not liable to be attacked. They require to be cryptically coloured so as to be able to approach their prey unobserved. The sexes are similar, therefore males are not espe- cially destroyed, and polygamy does not occur. Night Birds.— See Table II. These are, without exception, cryptically coloured ; attracting colour for the protection CONSIDERATION OF BRITISH BIRDS 109 of females and young would be useless, because at night it would be invisible to enemies. Birds of Song. — See Table III. In these birds, female protection is accom- plished by means of the attracting songs of the males. The young are protected by the parents rendering themselves conspicuous in other ways ; among these birds, the best examples of sacrificing and combative instincts and attracting motions and sounds, for the protection of young, are to be found. Other Birds.— See Table IV. The absence of conspicuous colouring in these birds cannot in all cases be explained. In the Divers, protection of young is not necessary, as these birds have always a re- treat at their feet — the water. Others are in reality birds of song, although not so placed, i.e. the Curlew, the Whimbrel, Redshank, Common Sandpiper, Greenshank, the Doves, and the Wryneck. Some inhabit thick under- growths, where conspicuous colours would not tell — Corncrake, Water-rail, and Hedge Sparrow. In the Partridge, the young are protected by attracting movements of the parents, which in this bird are remarkable ; but there seems to be no provision for the protection of the 110 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION female, except perhaps the habit the male has of always rising first, when the pair is flushed. In this bird, pre-copulatory displays are by no means wanting. There yet remain to be accounted for, the Greylag Goose, Sand- martin, Tree Sparrow, and Tree-creeper; some- thing might be said of each, but controversial and therefore doubtful facts would have to be utilised. DIVISION II This division includes birds presenting bi- sexual attracting coloration for the pro- tection of their cryptically, or much less brilliantly coloured young. Many of these birds have a seasonal moult : in the winter, when they do not possess young, they don a cryptic dress. In some the change from juvenile to adult plumage is slow, and extends over several years : usually these full-grown but not maturely coloured birds do not breed, and therefore do not require attracting coloration for protection of young. Further, adult and immature birds often keep together, in companies, for several seasons, in which case the gradual change from juvenile to adult plumage would be a prolonged ad- CONSIDERATION OF BRITISH BIRDS 111 vantage, in that young would be constantly protected, as in the Gannet. Although both the males and females of these birds are conspicuously coloured, the males are usually more conspicuous. This would make the males more powerful attrac- ting agents than females. Other methods are of course also utilised. There is evidence that some of these birds present warning coloration ; for instance, powerful birds (Swan, Cormorant, &c.), and ill-flavoured birds (Petrels and Kingfisher). DIVISION III Comprises birds having purposely con- spicuous or attracting coloration confined to the male. The vast majority of these birds (Table VII) make open nests, a habit that possibly causes the female to be cryptically coloured. Thus the male only has attractive coloration for the purpose of protecting the young. Arising in this way, this coloration is also used during the breeding season, for the protection of the female. In the analidae, the males usually take no part in the bringing up of the young, and yet the males are very conspicuously coloured during the breeding season ; moreover, directly CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION the males desert the females, they become, by a post-nuptial moult, cryptically coloured. It appears probable, therefore, that the male coloration during the breeding season is for the purpose of protecting the female. In the other birds, the males assist in the rearing of the young and do not lose this conspicuous plumage, by means of a post-nuptial moult. The following conceptions are thus arrived at: 1. The females are cryptically coloured be- cause of the open-nesting habits. 2. The males retain attracting coloration for the purpose of protecting the young. 3. The difference in colour between the sexes thus arising, is also used during the breeding season, for the purpose of protecting the female. Three birds which present sexual differences in colour, nest in closed nests : the Stonechat, Whinchat, and House Sparrow (Table VIII). In the case of the Stonechat and the Whin- chat, it is probable that this nesting habit is a recent acquisition, for they lay blue and blue-green eggs, characteristic of open-nesting birds, and not the white eggs usually laid by closed-nesting birds ; further, although these nests are domed, the covering is frequently CONSIDERATION OF BRITISH BIRDS 113 scanty ; and in the case of the Sparrow, may be entirely absent. The dome of the Sparrow's nest is never well-made nor very complete. It is necessary now to return to DIVISION II : " Birds having Conspicuous Colouring in Both Sexes." As would be expected, the majority of these make closed nests. There are, however, some which make open ones but whose females are not cryptically coloured (see Table V) ; in some of these the conspicuous colouring is concealed whilst the hen is sitting (Table V, * birds) ; others are powerful birds, or birds which nest in colonies and whose females are not liable to be attacked whilst sitting (Table V, birds marked f). Four birds, the Water-hen and the Coot, the Red- and Black- throated Divers, are able to leave their nests unobserved, and do so at the least alarm, so that it is very difficult to view these birds sitting. In two birds, the Lapwing and the Oyster- catcher, the male keeps guard over the sitting female and warns her on the approach of danger ; she is thus able to leave the nest when the enemy is far distant. Without opera glasses, it is difficult to view these birds whilst sitting. H CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION For these several reasons, it is not necessary for the females of the above birds, although making open nests, to be cryptically coloured. This completes the list of British Birds, with the exception of the Phalaropes and Dotterel, in which the female sex is the more brightly coloured one. In these birds, the male in- cubates the eggs in an open nest, and so only the female retains the conspicuous colouring that is used for the protection of young. One other fact is of some interest: many cases have been observed of females assuming male plumage; this change of plumage has been found to be associated with sterility ; whatever the mechanism by which this is brought about, it is clearly, according to the theory, an advantage to the species. Males are more conspicuous than females because they are less valuable, therefore a sterile female, because she is less valuable than a fertile one, becomes conspicuous. It will be well to review the facts which the theory correlates regarding the colour of British Birds : it accounts for 1. The presence of conspicuous colouring in both sexes. 2. The more conspicuous colouring of the male in these birds. 3. The presence of conspicuous colouring in the male only. CONSIDERATION OF BRITISH BIRDS 115 4. The absence of conspicuous colouring in preying birds. 5. The absence of conspicuous colouring in night birds. 6. The association of open- nesting with conspicuous males. 7. The post-nuptial change of plumage in the analidse and its absence in other epigamic coloured birds. 8. The brighter colouring of the female in the Phalaropes and Dotterel. 9. The presence of cryptic colouring in females and young birds. 10. The older the bird, the more brightly coloured it is, as in the Gannet. 11. The female assuming male plumage, when rendered sterile by disease. Thus can this hypothesis be applied to any selected material. In birds, it seems that protection of young is the most important factor in causing colour-differentiation in the society ; had insects been chosen, the protec- tion of females would have been seen to be a most important factor in sexual differentiation of colour. When material is considered in this way, that is, exhaustively, discourse is confined almost entirely to exceptions. A wrong im- 116 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION pression of the utility of the theory is thereby obtained, because it appears necessary to so frequently strain it. Theories are usually substantiated by chosen facts. It is well, therefore, to state that 173 birds have been considered, and only a few have required to be reviewed. TABLE I.— BIRDS OF PREY Falconidae - Marsh Harrier . . Hen Harrier . . Montague's Harrier Buzzard .... Golden Eagle . . White-tailed Eagle *Sparrow Hawk . Osprey .... Peregrine Falcon . Merlin .... Kestrel .... ~, .. f Richardson's Skua Stercorarimae \ ~ I Common JSkua Heron . Circus seruginosus C. cyaneus C. cineraceus Buteo vulgaris Aquila chrysartus Haliartus albicilla Accipiter nisus Pandion palisetus Falco peregrinus F. sesalon Tinnunculus alaudarius S. crepidatus S. catarrhactes Ardea cinerea [* Mimicked by cuckoo, Cuculus canorus.] TABLE II.— NIGHT BIRDS f Barn Owl . . Long-eared Owl Strigidse I Short-eared Owl Tawny Owl . . v Little Owl Strix flammea Asio otus A. brachyotus Syrnium aluco Athene noctua CONSIDERATION OF BRITISH BIRDS 117 Snipe . Woodcock . Stone Curlew Nightjar Bittern Gallinago caelestis Scolopax rusticulus (Edicnemus scolopax Caprimulgus europceus Botaurus stellar is TABLE III.— SONG BIRDS Missel Thrush . Song Thrush Nightingale Whitethroat . . Lesser Whitethroat Garden Warbler . Dartford Warbler Chiffchaff . Willow Wren Wood Wren Reed Warbler . Sedge Warbler . Grasshopper Warbler . Wren .... Meadow Pipit Tree Pipit . Rock Pipit . Skylark Woodlark . Corn Bunting Turdus viscivorus T. musicus Daulias luscinia Sylvia cinerea S. curruca S. hortensis Melizophilus undatus Phylloscopus rufus P. trochilus P. sibilatrix Acrocephalus streperus A. phragmitis Locustella nsevia Proglodytes parvulus Anthus pratensis A. trivialis A. obscurus Alauda arvensis A. arborea Emberiza miliaria TABLE IV.— OTHER BIRDS Hedge Sparrow . Spotted Flycatcher Sand Martin Tree Sparrow Accentor modularis Muscicapa grisol Cotili riparia Passer montanus 118 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION Tree Creeper Wood Pigeon Stock Dove . Rock Dove . Turtle Dove Whimbrel . . . Curlew Common Sandpiper Redshank . Greenshank Great Crested Grebe . Little Grebe Common Guillemot Greylag Goose Wryneck Corncrake . Partridge . Red Grouse Ptarmigan . Water-rail . Certhia familiaris Columba palumbus C. oenas C. livia Turtur communis Numenius phceopus N. arquata Tringoides hypoleucus Totanus calidris T. canescens Podiceps cristatus Tachybaptes fluviatilis Lomvia triole Anser cinerrus lynx torquilla Crex pratensis Perdix cinerea Tetrao scoticus T. mutus Rallus Aquaticus TABLE V *Red-legged Partridge . *Dunlin *Golden Plover . *Ringed Plover *Rlng Ouzel . *Kentish Plover . Lapwing Water-hen . Oyster Catcher . Coot . Black-throated Diver . Red-throated Diver Caccabio rufa Tringa alpina Charadrius fluvialis -<3Egialitis hiaticula Turdus torquatus A. cantiana Vanellus vulgaris Gallinula chloropus Hsematopus ostralegus Fulica atra Colymbus arcticus C. Septemtrionalis CONSIDERATION OF BRITISH BIRDS 119 Goldfinch . f Hawfinch . f Cormorant . fShag . fGannet fSwan . fSix Species of Gulls fFive Species of Terns . fRazor Bill . t Five Species of Corvidae f Black Guillemot . Carduelis elegans Coccothrauster vulgaris Phalacrocorax carbo P. graculus Sula bassana Cygnus (olor) Laridae Laridae Alea torda Uria grylle TABLE VI Hirundo rustica Chelidon urbica Muscieapa atricapilla Cinclus aquaticus Ruticilla pho3nicurus Erithacus rubecula Montacilla lugubris Saxicola oananthe Swallow House Martin Pied Flycatcher . Dipper- Redstart Robin .... Pied Wagtail Wheatear . Five Species of Parinae Gold Crest Wren Nuthatch . Starling Three Species of Corvidae Swift .... Cypselus apus Three Species of Picidae Kingfisher . Merganser . Goosander . Sheldrake Regulus cristatus Sitta caesia Sturnus vulgaris Alcedo ispida Mergus serrator M. merganser Padorna cornuta Four Species of Turbinaris Pufiln. Fratercula artica 120 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION TABLE VII Eider Duck . Mallard Shoveller Teal . Pochard Golden Eye Gadwell Garganey . Tufted Duck Widgeon Pheasant Capercaille . Black Grouse Greenfinch . Siskin . Chaffinch . Linnet Twite . Lesser Redpoll . Bullfinch . Yellow Bunting . Crossbill Reed Bunting Blackbird . Cirl Bunting Bearded Tit Blackcap Grey Wagtail Red-backed Shrike Yellow Wagtail . Somateria mollissima Anas boscas Spatula clypeata Querquedula discors Fuligula perina F. clangula Chaulelasmus streperus Q. cercia F. cristata Mareca penelope Phasianus colchicus Tetrao urogallus T. tetrix Ligurinus chloris Chrysometris spinus Fringilla co3lebs Linola cannabina F. flavirostris L. rufescens Pyrrhula Europcea Emberiza citrinella Loxia curvirostra E. schcaniclus Turdus merula E. cirlus Panurus biamicus Sylvia atricapilla Motacilla melanope Lanius collurio M. rayii CONSIDERATION OF BRITISH BIRDS 121 TABLE VIII Stonechat . . . Pratincola rubicola Whinchat . . .P. rubetra House Sparrow . . Passer domesticus TABLE IX Phalarope and Dotterel CHAPTER XII HUMAN AND OTHER SOCIETIES AT present only two societies have been dealt with, the Pair and the Family. Before, how- ever, passing on to others, a few remarks will be made relevant to the Pair among insects. In many insects the period of copulation extends over several hours and often over days. Moreover they are, during this period, quite defenceless and are able to utilise but poor means of escape. Therefore one would expect to find them exhibiting characters which would entice enemies to take males rather than females ; especially as the unnatural dis- joining of pairs, even a short time after copu- lation has begun, does not result in an entirely infertile female, but in a more or less com- pletely fertilised one. In some cases the separating of the pair takes place immediately either male or female are seized, and especially the male ; in others HUMAN AND OTHER SOCIETIES separation appears to be difficult or to require some time. Observations show that unpalatable insects especially have this habit, but too few have been made to justify a definite statement. If this prove to be a fact, its meaning is clear — they do not require this method for the escape of either male or female, because they are not so liable to attack. In this connection some observations, which have been made when birds are given copu- lating insects as food, have a bearing — one of the two copulating individuals is seized, but instead of both being at once swallowed, the bird for some reason almost always shakes or scrapes off the other. The impression gained is that the bird mistakes the second insect for an adherent foreign body. It appears, therefore, that were the male of a copulating pair to be seized by a pre- datory animal, there would be considerable chance of the female escaping. The following are methods of self-advertise- ment whereby male insects draw the attack of enemies : 1. Position. — Males rest on the top of the female and are therefore more likely to be seized. In many cases the males are very much larger than the females and completely CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION enclose them during copulation (the Cow-dung Fly). In 'other cases, although not on top of the female, the males are more ready to hand because the pair takes up a particular position in space ; for instance, the female may hang pendent from the male, this will encourage an enemy to seize the male : the bird would consider the point of suspension to be the head- end, and therefore seize this as being the most valuable part. 2. Movement. — When attempts at escape are made, it is almost invariably the rule that the male flies or runs carrying the female ; in order to prevent escape, the bird will invari- ably strike at the moving parts, the male : where there is movement there is vulnerability (many lepidoptera). Even when actual escape is not attempted, the male will often make attracting flutterings of his wings when dis- turbed; this is a very common character (most noctuce). 3. Colour. — Attracting colour in the male is common ; when both sexes are conspicuous the colour is usually " warning." 4. Mimicry. — It is common to find only females mimicking and thereby gaining pro- tection. The absence of mimicry on the part HUMAN AND OTHER SOCIETIES of the males renders them liable to be attacked. 5. Sound. — Males make sounds during copu- lation, especially when disturbed or captured. 6. Scent. — When both males and females are scented, the odour which they exhale is usually obnoxious and either advertises un- palatableness or renders them unpalatable. When males alone are scented they are per- fumed ; this renders them especially attractive rather than repulsive. 7. Dances are often made by males, who thus render themselves open to attack. The females do not leave cover except for a moment to join the males and be fertilised. These are pre-copulatory displays similar to those found in birds. Will not the dances of the Bower Birds, the running in and out of the bower, the display of bright objects on the ground around the bower, draw the attack of enemies and clear the air for copulation ? 8. Males seek the females, thereby expos- ing themselves to enemies more than females. It seems, therefore, that the Theory is able to explain the vast number of secondary sexual characters of insects. Other societies must now be considered, and first the Herd. Here, no new kind of indi- 126 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION vidual is found but a combination of sexually mature males and females, and young. This society differs from the Family, in that the onset of a breeding season does not break up the family formed at the previous breeding season. In this society, males are always much less numerous than females, and thus the inferior value of the male is demonstrated. Herd A, in which there are few males, is better off 'than Herd B, in which males and females are equal in number, because Herd A will require less of a limited food supply and yet have the same reproductive power. Now, probably in all vertebrate animals, the sexes are more or less equal at birth : the question therefore arises — How does the species rid itself of these males in excess of the number required, which are not only useless but positively harmful ? In the follow- ing ways : 1. By fighting among the males, the more powerful males destroy the less powerful. Herd A, whose males possess this instinct, will have an advantage over Herd B, whose males do not fight. 2. By the expulsion of males from the herd. The more powerful males, instead of destroy- HUMAN AND OTHER SOCIETIES 127 ing the less powerful, are sometimes satisfied by their banishment ; the females may assist after a battle has declared the victors. The expelled males, without the protection of the herd, probably quickly fall victims to preda- tory animals. 3. By the destruction of males by females : this is not unknown. 4. By enemies more frequently destroying males than females because of their possessing "attracting" characters. When a herd is attacked the males, because they possess com- bative and sacrificing instincts, usually protect the young and the females, and must often thus themselves be destroyed. If males and females were alike both in characters and instincts, as many females would be killed as males. By these methods, Natural Selection within the herd is controlled. The adult male pos- sesses special secondary sexual characters for purposes of offence and defence ; the young males are like the females and do not present these characters, these offensive and defensive instincts, until sexually mature — this because young males are more valuable than old ones. The attracting colour of male birds, which 128 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION live together in flocks, is not attained some- times for several years. Males, when at the zenith of their power, become monarchs of the herd for a season or two, and are then thrown out to die. Poor males never become monarchs. The per- centage of males cannot be dangerously reduced by these methods, because the per- centage of males to females at birth remains always the same. In certain species of insects, especially in the Hymenoptera, another kind of society is found, composed of males, females, young and neuters or asexual females. In these societies, the actual production of males is controlled by the females and neuters ; males are pro- duced only when required, and after they have fertilised the young females they either die or are destroyed, the females and neuters alone remaining to carry on the species. (The males of these insects are not provided with weapons of offence — stings, &c.) Here, then, pro- visions are made not only for the destruction of males but for the limitation of their pro- duction. Asexual reproduction in invertebrates, which is by no means rare, appears to be HUMAN AND OTHER SOCIETIES 129 for the same purpose, namely, in order that valuable food material may not be wasted on comparatively invaluable males. It seems that it is only owing to the necessity of occasional cross-fertilisation, that males are born at all. Finally, Human Society may be briefly examined in the light of the new Theory. The fighting instincts of the male are perhaps better developed in man than in any lower animal. Primitive races were perpetually at war and valuable females were the prizes of war. Races were prosperous or otherwise, according as they were good fighters or not ; this resulted in polygamy and great power of reproduction. To-day, the more civilised races do not fight and are therefore not polygamous. The reproductive power of the races of to-day is therefore much curtailed and the relative value of the female very much increased. Besides the fighting instinct, the male has a strong instinct to protect females even at the expense of his life, and both males and females have a similar instinct with regard to their children. The habit which the males of savage races have of rendering themselves conspicuous by paint and feathers, may be in i 130 CONTROLLED NATURAL SELECTION order to make themselves easily visible to their enemies, other tribes. It must be re- membered that savage man delighted in fight- ing; he did not seek always to take his enemy unawares, but rather desired to meet him in fair and open battle. There appears to be little in civilisation, to which this Theory can be applied. The present-day demand of the female for some recognition of her great value to the race, may possibly be a cry of Nature, a call from out the past. Human society illustrates the conclusions which were arrived at in Chapter IV by show- ing that competition does not lead to advance- ment, which is best gained by environmental differentiation or specialisation. Finally, it may be said that species rid them- selves of males in the following ways : 1. Sacrifice them to enemies. 2. Cause them to destroy one another. 3. Themselves destroy them. 4. Or control their production. Printed by BALLANTYNE, HANSON & Co. at Paul's Work, Edinburgh THIS BOOK IS DUE ON THE LAST DATE STAMPED BELOW AN INITIAL FINE OF 25 CENTS WILL BE ASSESSED FOR FAILURE TO RETURN THIS BOOK ON THE DATE DUE. THE PENALTY WILL INCREASE TO SO CENTS ON THE FOURTH DAY AND TO $1.OO ON THE SEVENTH DAY OVERDUE. OCT 2 0 1959 LD 21-100m-8,'34 321924 UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA LIBRARY