HIERARCHIAL BEHAVIOR IN THE SOUTH AFRICAN CLAWED FROG, Xenopus laevis Daudin ROBERT RICE HAUBRICH A DISSERTATION PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE COUNCIL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA August, 1957 She vrl-ter vlshes especially to aekncnrled^ Ms ladebtedaess to the late Dr. W. C. Allee and his vlfe, Mrs. W. C. Allee (Ann Silver), uhose ideas, encourageiaent aad advice vere Instrumental in the productioEi of this vork. fhe close personal relationship with both of than remains one of his most cherished oeBories. She writer also vlshes to Idiank Sr. B. Soffin Jbnes for taking on the hurdsn of this program after the death of Dr. Allee, and vlshes to adsxovled^ his indebtedness to him for eneouragem«nt and suggestions. Qratitude also goes out to Dr. H. M. Uallbrunn -vho gave advice on statistics. Dr. C. J. Gk>in vho suggested the e3q;>erltDental animal, Drs. L* * Bemer, J. H. Gregg, R. H. Waters and S. S. Wiaiberly vho criticized the text and to many fellow studaats Including C. D. Wilder, W. C. Sloan, C. D. Hynes, J. f . Howill and others for their discussions, critlclams and ideas relating to the developnent of this vork. 11 ■ . t'* * • ••• ...«« t i • 1 , k MKtezlals and IfethodA ^ Ssperlaattt X .«♦.•••••••••••♦♦••••*•• 3 Esperlaent H«,,##ji##fl#»«li(«»»» •• ^ Results « « 7 MSCUSSIOH OF laSSOiSBSB ..*••••••*••*««•#•• 30 gMMKY MD CQHCUJSIOii 38 l33CEtA3WKB CISBD • . • f. t # » i •♦•««#»♦**•• 1»0 BIOCBRAPHKAL I3B8 INTROIUCTIOR Hlerarchlal b«faavlor is a phenoaeam In indlvldu&ls display certain aggressive actlcns toward one another such as pecking or nipping. Differences in the frequency of these actions are often consistent enou^ to enable cue to rank individuals as to aggressive potential or position in relation to each other. Bvidaace of hiexwrcMal behavior Is fomd la such divergent parts of the animal kingdom as Arthropoda and Oiordata. Among invertebrates such behavior has been reported by Allee and Douglls (191*5) in the hermit crab, Pagurus longLcarpus. by Dou^s (l^) In the lobster, Hoaarus anerlcanus, by Bardli {19^) in the Tiasp, Pollstes galllGus, by Bwrjberg (1953) in the crayfish, Orooectes vinUs and by Lowe (1956) in the cre^yflah, Caaibarell\is shufeldtii. Aaong vertebrates evidence of hieararchial behavior is abundant in the class Osteichthyes. Braddock (19^5) demanstrated it In Platypqeellna aaculatus, as did Allee, Greenberg, Rosenthal and fraxik {19^) in the gireai eunfish, Leponis cyanelltxs. Hewnan's (195^) work gives infonnation on intrar and interspecific conrpetltion as well as hlerarchlal behavior in two trout ■pedes, aaloelinns fontinalls (Mitchell) and Salao gairdaeri Richardson. Evidence of hlerarchlal behavior in the class Reptilla was reported by CSreenberg and Noble (19^*) in the American CSxameleon, Anolls carolinensis. Members of the class Aves probably have been studied with the greatest frequency and have yielded most in the vay of facts and principles of hlerarchlal behavior. Papers by Schjeldenq>-a!bbe (1913* 1935) aad Quhl and AllM (X^) on th« dooestic tcnrl, Eeuoks (195^) m ,}tBigIe fowl, Mtibs^hlag (19^1) on plgeoas, B«mett (1939) on ring dores, Hovard and Anlen (19^2) on <|uall, HauBaerstram (l9'^2) on chickadees and Sabine (19^) m JtDieoa and tree aparrova Indicate the vide cov«M0t in -^a elasa on Tjoth dame sti Gated and vild forms. . .j, . . Wbrk on rats "by Uhrich (1938)^ aqolrrels "by Gordon (l9kO), infra- htanaa primates by Maslov (I9U0), chlarpanssees by Crawford (19^2) and Yerkea (19^ ^ Rhesus laanlaay by Carpenter (19^2) all demonstrate the social hlerardiy in the class MaBiaalla* A form of hlerarehial organlssatlaa In htman Indlviaaal and grottp l)rtucvlar Is Indicated by HasMns (1951)* Allee (1951), Keith (l9*^9), Sherlf (1956) and other*. Hhile 'Uiis phenotaenon reaches Its grtfttwit eotoplexlty aoong verte- brates (Allee, 1952) mtll recaitly no hlerarcMal behacslor had be«i re- corded far the classes Agoatha, Chondrlchthyes or An^pliibla. Bie first breajt 111 this Bttuation occiirred vhen Allee and lUcldLnaon (l95*^) reported finding a primitive form of this behavior in the dogfish ehark, Masteliis eaalSi a member of the class Chmdrlehthyes. !Qais leaves A^gaa-ttxa, regarded as the most primitive vertebrate daaa, «aA ih^phlbla, a claaa of medlm coBq?lexlty as the oaaly two elaaaes of Vertebrates in which there has been no report of hlerarehial b^unrior, Prtfvious works have Indicated aggresslcn end territoriality in tSie class Aatphibia, but not hlerarehial behavior. Martof (1953) reported on a sort of territoriality observed among breeding males of the gre«i tros, Bana clamltapis, based on Isolation, a type of intolerance, advertiseaoent and fixation. He augcgeated tbat a relatively aggreaaive perioa is followed by one of stability, lAloh ColllaB (19^) cmsldered to b« the case In oth«r vertebrates. Pearson (19^^) found in the spadefoot toad, Scaphiopus boIbroQki holbroolgl (Harlan), avoidance reactions smoag Individuals and a type of tmdef ended home site territory vhlch seldom <9rerlapped others even in crowded areas. ^Test (195^) reported the first definite case of aggresslca In the class Auzphlhla as displayed iA dtffnat o£ ttzxitarjr "by the frog, Phylobates trinltatis Boulaiger. Bvldence of territoriality In the bull* frog, Rana catesbelaoa, was reported by 0. B. Ctoln (1955 )• ^ ^& vas observed to croak and jm^ at a smaller frog In the same general area vhlch resulted in the retreat of the smaller animal. Jn addition, other indi- viduals vere consistently fo^{nd in certain areas located around a pool* Qrant (l955) also reported on a case of territoriality in two species of salamanders, Eurycea bislineatja and Hemidactyllum scutatua, based upon a #»haniytep end a defended territory, tlie smm of defense included direct biting attack in B. bisllneata and challenge only in H. scutatum. Preliminary experlofints were attempted on certain species of frog« but the problaa of classifying the actions of the animals yblle under obser- vation was difficult. Use of Xenopus laevis Daiadin as the e^qperimentzQ. animal was suggested by C. J. Goin (1953) of Ifalversity of Florida because of its habits and mode of life. !Ilhe purpose of this study was four- fold: to determine ^diether hierarchial behavior was present in Xenopus, to analyze the phencnKUXi of kLeocarchlal behsvloor, to fill a gap in r«qi>oart8 of hierarchial b^vlor In dlfflerent ph^ogeneHo groops and to add to 13m» information on aggression with possible applications to human behavior. MfcterlaXs and Methods Frogs tised in ^bi» study weare obtained from the sttpoply house of Jay E. Cook, CoclmeyBvllle, Maaryland and were received as adults by Cook from South Af^ca (see Walker, 19^2)* Parlor to the observatioa period animla vere kept in five gallon aqpiaria filled with water to a depth of ten inches. Ihey vere kept out of direct sunlight and t«iiperatures vere maintained between 6cPand 75° F. Bie vater was changed after each feeding or vh«i it became doudy. At first city water which had been conditioned by four days storage was xised, but after sane miwAl » died Xooal spring '^r'H feet that peimlt rapid B»veineat in any direction, a well-developed lateral line systeni (Slkan and Ifurray, 19^ araionia excretion in adult (Muaro, 1953), and nasal cavities lined with sensory epithelixm that functions In olfsotozy perception ^en submerged (Bat«r«oa^ 19$l)* ^ spite of the aquatic habit at times the is seen ntcnrlng orerlmd or is found In clods of BWd near dried up ponds (Leslie, I89O; Rose, 1950). Two groups of twelve female frogs each were used in this study and were kept in 8 - 10 inches of water at all times. ObservBtions on frogs 1 > 12, desigoated as Ebgoeriment I or Ckroup I took place in the Marine Bio- logical Laboratory at Wbods Hole from June to August 195^* Observations on frogB ion - 112, desismted as Sxperiment II or Oroizp II, were coorpleted duiing the smmr aad fall of 1955 ^'t the University of Floricla. - . . Ibqperlinent I ftLx frogs iwre kei^t betwMSi observatioaa la MOh of two tea-galloa aquaria and vere wlsShed once a veek duriag the eaperiaeatal period (Table l). When animals were fir»t received certain digits on the froait limbs were clipped to facilitate reeogaltiott. Further clipping vas unnecessary, even thou^ digits grev l>ack, ■beca\i8e characteristics like size, color pattern, behavior and body shape varied nldaly anong iadlvidualB and awds it easy to recognise then alter they had been observed for a time. Pairs vere observed separately in five -gallon aquaria. Before each series of pair observations vater -was changed and t«fo different animals vere used. During all observations cxibes of liver, about l/h Inch cn a side, vere plB«ed in aquaria oae at a tine as the animals ate them. Feeding occurred cBily during time of dbserration. ^ . Activity differ«ices aiaong individuals were noted by pairing frogs In all possible combinaticnB. Each pair vas oibaarved a total of 200 ainutes divided Into ten twenty minute periods. Biese ofbserration periods nwre con- ducted in two sets of five each. Periods vithin each set followed each other consecutively, excepting short breaks between periods for lib* «aiperlmenter to rest, but the two sets Involving one pair of animals vere not run consecutively. 3Dbie followlag infoimation vas recorded during the pair observations for each 1. Hips given. A nip vas recorded when one frog nipped or bit anotiier and caused the other to retreat* 2* Pushes glvei. A puBh was recorded vhea oaae frog appcroacbed another and displaced It throu^ contact. ^ 3. J^^oaches* Ifunber of times each frog approached other aotloaLess rwBiber of pair aad touched -without further activity. k, I&toowa. Number of times the two aalaals made contact, vhlle in maticn, vlth uo dbserTable dlffereatial betwem th«BU 5. Cuib«B of liver eaten "by eaeih ftfog and th* ordBr in iiblch each was obtained. 6. !Ehe troQ obtaining food first at the start Of tioh 8«ri«8. Activity differences were also noted vhaa each frog was isolated a total of 200 minutes ^ttvlded as in liie paired sltuatlcn into two series of ten twenty-Bdnute periods each. Olhe following infonaatiOBi was recorded for each firog while isolatedi 1. KtBober of cttbes of liver satsn^ : 2. Hoe in seconds talaen to obtain each piece of liver. . ■ Ejqperlment IZ ■ 2h IMs eaqperlment, in order to provide better control and prevent possible carryover of hierarchial behavior, the frogs were kept isolated in OM ffallGBi jttrt bstuMR obMOvations. i^alred and isolated animals were ob« sttred using nsthods similar to tluMM in Ixperlment Z. Temperatures varied between 71° - 7^° ?. and the aniioals were welgjxed weekly during pair obser- vatioas (OUile lA). In Experiosnt ZZ^ to dAterain* ^-Oisr poeitlcn in the hierarchy was consist^t under different eondltlQaB, observatlaDis wer* asds on sach "Tnima^ vhen in a groig> of twelve as well as \dien in a pair cooiblnatiai. Since it vae li^posslble to record the activities of all the frogs in the aguariuffi simultaneously the behavior of eaeth frog with reference to the other eleven vas observed for a total of 200 minutes. Each flrog "was mdnr ohservatlcEi for a total og 200 wln^iteii 9oA OfHif •ffbf.vities involving the tingle frog "being observed at -ttie moBtent irere reecaraed. For instance, no record was kept of idiieh animals were nipped by or received nips from the frog under observation. Observations vere made like those on the paired situation, namely, in ten i20HBinute periods at the rate of five periods a (laar* 3he animals vere laolated a mtnlTiniin of twelve hours after each dally observation • •- •• • ■• ..■ '. • ■■ ' ' ■ ■• - Besaltg ' ' febles 1 and lA indicate restdts of weekly weijjiings in number of grams. Individuals in Group I varied in average veldts frca ^0 to 168 gnats, while individuals in Group II varied frcoi 37 to lUo grazas. All of Qratx^ I (frogs 1 • 12) showed some gEdn during the experim^ital period as did all but three frogs in Gtck^ II (frogs 101 - 112). Vhen animals were placed together in pairs one of four different tjGPes of ac-U.vity wu <^s«rved9 1. e. the nip, puah, approach or unknown. These types of acttrlty ftccrlng emtacts wee recorded for each pairing and the results are shown in t&ables 2 throii^ 5« Besults in the nip category are recorded in Sable 2 for frogs 1 - 12, and in liable 2A for frogs 101 • 112. Animals were ranked in order of total nips given in all pairings. In Baperlment I this varied from UQ to 279; while In Experim^t II it varied from 3 to ^77* lianklng of frogs for total nips givei (totals along the bottom row) was correlated to ranking for total nips recelred (totals along the left coUna}* Frogs X • XS v»ri«d in total maaber of nips received from 76 to S^h. Hfhm. individual reoikl&gs in naaber of nips given were cooipared to ranldngs in nuiaber of nips received the results showed a ccarelation of -O.96. Frogs 101 - 112 varied in total maiber of nips received dftom 57 to iSk, and -when individual rankings in nuniber of nips glwi veare compered to twnliingB In nips received the result was a correlation -ed to rankings in appro- aches received in Experiment X tdiae result vas a ccnrrelatioaa of -0*83, and in Ibqperimesat II the same ccmpartsons produced a correlation of -0.88. Id. Tfehle k, 1*2 of the 66 pairings showed statistically sigiificant differences, indicated "by asterisks, "betWMn individuals in nianber of approaches made, while in Table kA, 32 of the 66 pairings showed this* l . - ... SHe untoaoua category, represented hy Tables ^ and ^ indicated the otBlber of ttms trofft nade cmtacts that -were of equal or of no aggressive potmtial as nearly as the observer could determine. The nttmbers of con- taots vere the same for each meciber of each pair heoauae both Bniml.s vere given credit vhaiever a ccaatact of this type occurred. Tbe total nvoiber of ccmtacts in the unknown category among individuals varied from 667 to 2525 in Sscperlment I and from 511 to 2li-ll in Experiment II« ■..^> In Tables 9 and 9A total food consUKcption, as measured by cubes of liver eatea, is recorded far each frog in each pair combination. Ihe animals vere rsnked according to total ntaaber of evOftes eaten ov«r all pairings. Sable 9 gives results for frogs 1-12 and shows an individual variation in food consuaQytian from I5I to 327 unite. ISible 9Jl gtves results for frogs 101 - 112 and the food ccnstmiptlan in tMs group varied fjram 75 to 803 units. When the rankings in total amount of food consuoed by the other eleven fjrogs labile paired vlth each frog vere compared to rankings in total amount of too^ eaten "by each £rog^ correlatloas q£ -0*62 atid •, respectively). ■ The number of times each f^g obtained food first at the start of a series of observations wts rtcorded in OSable 11. la the paired situation individuals varied from 4 to I6 in this category. When the group of twelve animals vera compered a variation £raa 0 to U nas observed. Individual variation in av«rage anount of tine taken to eOytalA food •while isolated was recorded in teble 12. !I5ie frogs in Bxperiment I varied txaa, 72 to 768 seconds vhile those animals in Esperlment II which ate varied from to 13^ seconds, but two of the animals in Experiment H ate no food idiiXe Isolated. 'QBable 13 shows rankings in each of the categories for frogs 1-12 These rankiass to be lised in correlatioai tests (Table l^i-) were obtained frcm the following tables! Hips vhile pair«d • . . . • Tables 2, 2k Poshes ^^lils paired * fables 3j 3^ Appcroaches -while paired Tables k, kk UaJfflown wMle paired ..*••••«»«* Sables 5> 3k Etps while in groups of twelve ««•••* Table 6 ..... Pushes -while in fiewtps of -tarelve ..... Table 6 - Awage -wei^t •»•••••••••••• Tables 1, lA Food consump-tion -lAile in pairs Tables 9t 9k Urn taken -to locate f ood vhile Isola-bed . Table 12 Table ik shovs the cCO i»t r4 b- 1- 0\ t» * 01 tn * u\ coco t-vo rooivo-* •<*?o. ^ • H H irwO trxirv t— OS VO J* ONJt O • ONCO H • *. t • H H H W H 57 H-sf 01 >o iiNO| t<-^ o\oo (n 5 . I 15 1| a I 5 I 1 n I g " I . f ft <3 X O ^ fl^ t I « t t- ^-^J> t>-^ »^ vo CU trv O O u>v£) H • ON r|j ij^***;:} #*4 iA-^ ^ t- ITv CO v5 ra CO I « a >• o o a 5 ^1 « § •d IP S ^ 2l ^ o 1 I I I I 1 I 2 HHHWH H * H ^ *v 5i '^^ 'lOt-itnH tn UMfWO o TABES 6 mSSBBR OF NIPS AUS posses giver ASD KECEZVED BI each mXi VSES Frog Nips QivmL Pteoeived Pushes CbLven Secttlv*cl Rtps-Pushes Giveu nps-Pushtts Becelved 101 102 103 10^ 105 106 107 108 109 no 111 112 Sbtal 13 39 12 a6 7 19 35 8 53 19 13 38 Ik 89 33 1^ 36 Ik 5 19 193» ' ' 537* 735* 58 19»t 30«* g ll 850 ''.5 iff 928 # • Total nips or pushes given in this case do not necessarily ecfoal total I nips or pushes received laecause in the grouped sitoEition fro^s were observed one at a time. Ho record vas kept of vhicb frogs gave nips or pushes to or received aipe or puahea ixoa. tJie particular aninal being observed. Ck»rx«latlax (r^y) b«tw»ca nuogibcr o^* aijpfl given and nips received vas -0,k3 Onnrtlatlon (r^y) between mafter of poaibes etvea txdt poshes received vas -0.62 SABXS 1 TREQMEHCX OF EACH CQRZACT TXPE WBEIT fROGS WERK IS PAIRS Hugiber Observed Per Cent of Total Type of Cmtact Experiment E:!iperlment I n I EC Mp Push Approach 1552 2691 9033 lllM im 1890 5239 9527 6A 37.0 >5.6 28.9 52.5 mai ^ik20 laiuo 100.0 100.0 ■ V. naftlMJi OF EACH C(MEACT TZfE WHEN FROGS WERE IN GRCXJP OF THELVS lype of Ckmtact JSmSoer Observe .■ Hip 506 Ptuh 1272 Ibtal 1778 CNON HH H H H H H CW • « LA!IED^ PAIRED AHD GROUPED Sllf lATICMIS COHSDERED IN TAH2 10 Ccnrparlsoci t P< Isolated -Paired 0.k2 0.7 Isolated-Grouped 3.9T 0.01 Paired-Grouped 2.69 0.05 TABOE U nUHBSR OF a?IMES EACH TBOG OBTAIMD FOOD FIRST WHE MBQB OP A PAIR - EXPERIMBHTS I MP 11 - AND WHILE IN A GROUP OF THEL7S SXFXRIMDiT II Frog I I I 9 10 u 12 Paired Frog 101 102 103 10i» 105 106 lOT 108 109 HQ 112 10 13 9 8 U 10 17 Ik 15 6 3 Ik 6 i» 3 7 6 3 0 1 0 « # u Total 132 130 k5 TABtB 12 ATEBAGB HlMBaSR OF SECONDS TAKES BY FROGS WHSTJ IS0LA3EEID (TO OSCAIK EACH OF FOOD > EXFSRIXEIfIS I AHD H Frog Average Tlste Frog h. . . -ii^: ■ . . 101 e 7» 102 3 768 103 k 68 l(A 5 7k - 105 6 157 106 I 1^ 107 220 108 9 137 109 10 130 no n 83 HI 12 118 112 Average Time lEABES 13 RAHEDIGS OF FE«X}S IH YARIOOS CAEEGORISS SmLRIMESTS X MB 11 Frog Hip Puah Ajfproaeh Haknown Hip Push Grouped Food Food Eat«i Tijae Weltftt % I I I 9 U 3. t I X I i I 9 9 n 20 10 9 i U T 1 i s 5 9 10 u 8 6 7 EXnOPJMEHT I 8 2 12 12 1 5 1 f 1 19 u 11 10 8 9 1^ 7 12 3 5 10 7 6 U in 109 lU i i I I 10 u U 12 9 9 t I 6 2 7 5 1 l^ 9 12 11 10 EXPSRIMEaiT n f i u 12 10 I 5 9 ! k u 12 10 8 9 7 X I f 9 3 k 10 11 12 a 2 I 2 2 12 5 6 i lABlX Ik BARE CORBELmORS 07 TSS CMEOOIRZSS VTIB IBSfS IIIDICAIIlia PBOBABILm OF CORBELATIOH Mim AS LARCS OR LABGSB SUE TO CHANCE AUSM (Edwards, 195k) CoQcparlsoaxs Frogs 1-12 from 101 - 112 Correlatioa P< Correlatlm P < O.QL 0.8l 0.01 .70 .01 ,ek .01 .ko •05 •32 .3 .77 .€0. .IS •a * •Ql • • • • • • • • .71 .01 • • • • .76 • • • • • • • « .97 .oe .85 .1 .90 ,6k V .09 .87 •03. .56 .1 .76 .8e .03. .27 ■ .73 .OL .36 .83 .01 .52 : .1 .06 .52 ^ .91 .67 .02 .80 ' m ,1 .80 .08 .d .63 m « .73 •01 .26 .10 .8 .70 .38 .3 .28 .1 KLp/Puali Nip/Appapoada Push/Approach Nip/ltotaiowa Push/lSikncnm Approach/UbloioHii Paired Grouped y ItLp Hip Puah Push Hp ' Grouped Nlp/Ghra>i^d Push Nip/FOod Cmsvmtptiaa Posh/Food Consiairptiaa Approach/Food Cosisisiipticxi tfaJmown/Food Coasuntptloa Hip/Food Zine Push/Food Time ' Approach/Food SiiM tiaiaxown/Tood !Eba« Hip/WBiejxt Push/Weig^it Approacli/Wei Uokaown/Veic^t Food Consuniptioai/Wel^t Food Slme/Wei^t ©ISCUSSIOH OF KESUiaS 'iOils study is coQcexned vlth three basic probLns all dealing vlth hierarohial 'bsbxTiors the behavioral display of aggrsssion in fjrogs, indi- vidual differences as to aggressive behavior and factors related to indi- vidual differences in agsnMisiQti. She statistically sigoificant eorrelatiocs that are se«a in flible 1^ vhen ranMngs of frogs in tlie nip^ push and approach categories are com- pared support the assimptiaa that thett |j)^x«e categories represcsxt different expressions of the same thing, namely, aggression. In addition, the hierarchial standing of frogs 101 - 112 In -Qie nip and push categories as indicated tiaa pali^Lngs are higjily cozrelated vlth hierarchial positians of the MBM frogs \di«a observed in a group of twelve (Tlables 6 and lU). fhus, frogs nipping the laost also posh most nbether they are observed in successive pairs or as mflsxbers of a group of twelve. Correlations between rankings in the uaknoun and aggressive cate- gories are generally much lower than correlations among razUdngs in each of the aggressive categories (Hkble l^i'). Hhis is evidence tlxat the unlsnown category is different and It laay include aggressive activity of a low level, it also indtides aotivltieB of a noai-ftggr«ssive nature. suable 7 shovs the frequencies of the nip, push, approach and uixknoun categories t^en frofs wur* saired. 3Sm unkncwn oatetfary oalEM up about ^0 per cent of all contaets, ■wifliat ovttr 30 per eent of all eoataets shov a veak foim of aggression referred to as the approach, a little more than 10 per cent shov definite aggression in the push and sonetdiat less than 10 per emt lndle«t« the aost IntaaiM farm of aggressloa, namely, the nip. Sables 6 and 8 give frequencies of the nip and push oategories vhen frogs vere in a grcn;^ of twelve* Tbm relative frequoicles of nips end pushes are of the MM order ^diether animals are paired or In a group of twelve. A negative relationship exists between intensity of aggression and its freq^ieney of eapresslon i^ther trogfi are observed in a series of pairings or in a groi^ of twelve. . Each group of firogs dlcqplaysd a rather loose foim of hierarchy based tQKBx differences ooDcag individuals in aggressive pot«itlal as measured by the nip, push and approach. Sables 2-3 show the structure, arrangonent •ad extsnd of these individual differences. Wide variaUons among frogs are Indieated y/hm all aggresslTS aetirity is totaled for each amlnal. XUfferenoes in aggressive activity between individuals in any one pair are noted by cooqpering totalis for each grog within the chart* flftgr pcx* cent of the differttxces between members of pairs are statistically significant as indicated by asterisks in the squares of the greater totals. Ttae per cent of sl0ilfleaatly dlfferwit totals remains consistent for both groups of frogs tested and also for the thzve aggressive categories. Bank oorrelation between total aggressive activity esipressed and total aggressive activity permitted in this study is negative and statistically significant. Frogs with the bluest aggressive potential, as measured by sudaer of nips, pushes and approaches administered, eaperleooe the tmm9% a^resslve advances from other frogs and vice versa. IMs sugsssts IStat aggression Is a distinct foxm of behavior and is based upon certain behavioral cues not detected by the observer. c-;,.,;' ' Oi^er iafoxnatloti recorded during periods of Q^wrmfeiais Included the follotfiug: (l) wei^ts, (2) amounts of food eaten aaid coBrparisons vh«i fJrogs imre isolated, paired and grouped, (3) time taken to obtain food virile iscdbted and (k) nximber of times food v&s obtained first vhen indi" viduals were in pairs or in a grovq? of twelve. . ^. TablA Ik aHaam correlatloas between all of the faxrbors. It Is trca -tiwse eorrelatlaas, Ixued upoa rankings of the frogs (Table that most interpretations regarding relatlonsJaipe are made. • < " t Weigjit dlffereoces sd^t be thou^t of as one ejigpressloa of physio- logical variation. When rankings in wel^t, obtained fraa Zeble 1, are correlated with rankings in the nip, push, approach and imknown categories the resvdts are variable (Skble 1^}. Differences noted previously betwMm aggressive bdsavlor and the unknown category carry throu^ whezi the wei^t factor is considered. Four of the six correlations between the aggressive i«tgtgoirles and wel^t are statistically sl^ilfleant and the othar two are almost 80. All are positive. Velght is considered to be a factor la bierarchlal behavior of Umam frogs and heavier froigs tend to rank hlgber in the three aggressive categories, niBMly, vlp, puih. and approach, yet do not necessarily rank higgler in the uzxknown category. ffehles 9 and 9A show how frogs rank In individual food caisuDptlaa. differences in food consvpption are not as clear-cut as those within each of the aggressive categories. Less than ^0 per cent of the pairings show significant differences between frogs as Indicated by asterisks within the tables. Also, tAien rankings of frog? in total food consvaoed (totals in bottoa row) are caqpared to ranldnga in total food coasuned by others in their preseace (totals In rl^t coltnm) Mbe results are Inconclusive. Group I shows a slgEdfleantly hlgjti eoarrelatloaa, Grotrp II does not, althouf^, all correlations are negative. , : ^ ISmq renMngs in vel^t are ccccpared to rankings in aiaount of food eaten the results are variable. ^Bm heaviest animals In Group II tend to •ftt Urn most vherefts the heanrlest iu Qrouip I do not neeasflarlly •at tb* aost. ^hen ranMngs In tooA emstnptlQn are eorrelstttd tdlb thyote In three aggressive categories and the unlmown the results are statlstiC6Llly signif- icant at the 5 per cent level In six out of el^t cases and close In the raaalnlng two. dere Is no T>reak In degree of correlation observed here, aa In previous cocrparlsans, "between the three aggressive categories and the vcbkamti, ■which indicates the more aggressive as well as the niore active frogs eat more than the less aggressive and less active animals. Sie srezase tljae ta]fiea l>y isolaiied aaiOAls to Iocftt« food varies vldely anong inttvlduala (Wble 12). Qmm dlffWrwices May ^ to variations In the chemoreceptor systems and in ability to respond to -ttiese stimuli. When rankings in this category are cooipared to rankings in the aggressive categories (Table ill-) the correlatians, while all positive and significant at the 5 per cent level In Orovq} I, are positive "but of questloaaa'ble sigalflcenee in Group H. When rankings in aioount of time taken to locate food are ccccpared to rankings In amount of food consmed th« coi7elation« are not consistently hi£^. Sh^refore, the xoore aggressive firogs do not neeesswrlly find fbod more galekly, nor do the frogs eating more while paired necessarily take the least time to locate food \iben. iso- lated, t.; ■ , . . ■■■ IxifQaxmtim aa the nusber of tljaes each fjrog obtained food first >)btle a noftier of a pair and vhlle In a grotq) of tvel'V« Is gtrsa In Table U. Whidx animal obtains food first at the start of each series of ohser- vatloos is apparently a chance phenorattion not noticeably related to any factors previously discussed. . 'v • - It Is possllale that the three aggressive categories represent attempts by frogs to eat one another. Cannibalism Is veil knona In this species especially -whan size differences are great and availability of food Is limited (Rose, 1950). 2he behavior resulting in a alp vas observed to be very sladlar or Identical to that leading to constnspticEi of Uver. Osily the size and movement of the other frog semed to prevent the coanpletion of the act of eating In the case of the nip. Ihe Increase in frequencies of nips, pushes and approaches in the presaice of food is further evidence sufg^orting this asstsiptloii* If the observations noted above are valid the atteorpt to obtain food laay censtltute a camnoa basis for aggression in laaagr vertebrates. ;•. ,• ■ ■ . ;'; A short tis» afber a tult of food Is plao«4 in an a|yis^ a oMrtaln roluae of vater, mA In adflltion iSbm greater amount of Infiiviaaal activity no doUbt requires tbe use of more energy. Tiahle 10 shows the compjurative aoounts of food eaten by frogs viaea isolated, vbesa. paired and •whan In a group of twelve. Hhe total amount of food consumed by frogs varies directly Kith the nuniber of animals plafted togetlxir. Differences in total food con- suinpUm betweei the isolated and pairiA iltuatians are not staUstically Biffiificant, but tihen ftogs are observed in groups of twelve the total con- sanption is slgilflcanUy greater than in either of the other two situaUons. If increased food cansumptlcn is actually of poslttve value to the anJaal, this may be an esaiQtle of cooperation or the Allee Effect (Odum, 1953) • •Che present work has indicated the existence of certain b^wviaral «itlties by the method of analysis. It has also attcaapted to relate these entities one to aaother and relate thsm to certain broader aspects of living things ouefe as iihyelology, agg»»iim, copperaUon and hlerarchlal behavior. An atteimrt at controlled observation is used here to facilitate an mderstandlng of behavior both In one species and as a ganeral phenomenon of life. While one person has been studying the brtiavlor of these frogti the data as tabulated In a asnse have reflected certain aspects of human behavior. On the whole, correlations are hlijier in (kna^ II taaan they are in Gxoiq* I, when rankings in the three aggressive categories and -ttie unknow «re coogpared to each other and to rankings in other factors. It could be that frogs in Qmapa 1 and II vere behavlorally dlffenmtj on the other hand, as a result of continued work vlth the animals, the experimenter may have learned to fit certain aspecte of behavior into categcxries with greater ease in the second groi^). Assuaalng iatie latter, this probably represents greater accuracy tn dstexminlng wt^efseSjta In Qroup II. Hofverar^ there still is a source of danger in the metbod of analysis used in this study. Blvlslcsi of the bdiKvioral yboHA Into gui^ increasingly aGn>Bx«at categories or entities ■i^t eneeniMgi m eibeerver to igaore relatiaBdilp? between peu^s and the bduYioral vfaole. Cie parts versus the vhole dichotomy is one that science luMi AM9ed £rm the ,pt«rt, because an inqportant feature of the scientific ttetliod is the analysis of vholes into paarts. Often diehotaoies are eliffl* Inated as more is known about a subject. For Instance, the study of heredi-ty moA suTiroament has becosie not so imich a pcroblen of two separate things as different aspects of the same thing. ItLkeviBe, future vork may shov that hairarehial behavior is a special case of territoriality or vice versa. , . Mooh of this vort: bai esntemd around liw study of tadlvldual dif « ferences in aggression, i. e. the determination of the presence of and form Of biararchial }»tBmriM iA this £rog. An analysis of hierarchlal behavior is also attenipted. An increasing number of studies In animal behavior are not concerned vlth an analysis of both the phenomenon of aggression and of the hierairohy (Allee, Foreoan, ftuiks and Holablrd, 19?^; Braddodc, 19^(91 Bovjberg, 1956 j Crawford, 19l»0i Colllas, 19^3i Douglis, 19^) Gre«iberg, 19^7) and Delany, 1955)* Such studies njjmamt att«spts to analyze the broader aspects Of behavior. . - Many European workers, especially theebhologlsts, emphasize the miilywLa of more basic betuvrioral eatitios m is evident in the studies of releaser mechanisms (Tinbergen, 19^^^ 19^^ 19?3), social faeilitation (Barling, 1938j Davles, 1953)^ displacement activities (Lorenz, 19^1j Wnbergen, 1953) and imprinting (lorenz, 1952K . ; , Sbie cLLff erences vhlch exist betveea these tvo schools iu their mpfproaches to the geaeral study of behavior are also evident in their ^fffffcmahies to the study of bdiavloral evolutioa. Sils is clearly shovn tilum the Su3^^pean viewpoint discussed by tOEtRiz (19^0) Is coopered with the American airproach as Indicated by Allee (1952). ^Ebe Europeans attempt to coorpare oore basic bn^Msviorttl entities aiacxig dif£to«&t «gp«ol«8 KiiyBreas the Americans attenq^t a ccepearison of more general phenoBMna. Both astproachas are of course useful in any study of evolutiacL be it organic or behayloral. A study «M mdi of two groups of twelve frogs In an ftttesapt to amalyie «sgrossicin and to drtemLne ^Aetlier inOividaaU wlIMn the gjroi^ y [lY^an iggressive differeaces resultlxig in hlerarchial behavioor. 1» Aggressioo in Xgoopus laevis Tln^^^n aiJ^Ojaditid ■behavioraUy ia one of three ways, namely, "by the ajjproach, tflie pttflh sod the nip. These Obtteories also represeat iaoreasing intensities of aggressive behavior in tSie oraer given. 2* Contacts repires«iting the aore intense categories of aggresslTe 1»^hearlor occur le«9 ftsfMatiy ttm do tHoee r«(pres«atin6 Hio less inteoae ones in 'bath the paired and grcuped situatione. 3. Large differences aoang frogs are indicated vhen aggressive ecfcivity is totalled for each anlaal. Progs displaying 1^ aost aggressive activity in one category tend to display the aost In the others* k» Differences in «ount of aggressive "behavior displayed betwMtt airftan of pairs are statistically slgplfloant in ^ per cent of the cases. !Ihls is ccQsistent for each of Ha* aggressive categories. ft A category '^rispn*^ as unkoown repres«a.ts contacts in "which, no a^resslve SUfxm&m MmMI mOmXa could be detendned. She total frequency of these kinds of contacts is as great as all other types cooibined. 6. Frogs vlth greater aggressive yoteatials, M Msured by nuaber of nips, pushes and approaches, experieaace fewer aggressive advances flrom otiier fro^ and vice versa. 7« Seavler frogs display aggressive behavior aoore frequoatly than lighter caaes, yet are xxot necessarily 'tiM aore active anlasls (aotivity referrin^ to raoMixg in the untooua catee^ry). 8. Sbe more aggressive as imU as oore active £rog& eat more thaa the lees aggressive and less active anlneifl, ytt lAiiii renMngs in total food coasuoed are compered to raaldngs In total food consumed by others In their presence tiie results are sot necessarily Tal^oly correlated* 9. K> cousistoit relatlonehlps exist amoig rankinfls in fdod can- suB^tioti, tim l&ken. to locate food viiile Isolated and vel^t* IQ, As the nualber of anljoaals placed togttixer is increased to twelve, the amount of food ccxisuaed "by each troQ increases. !Ihls may "be an eTBMiila of cooperation or the AUm fiffeot* XI* It is possl^ thttt aggresiiiit« 'behavior in these fSrogs reflevto an attatrpt to eat (me anotberj as "behavior resulting in a nip is nearly identical to that vhich leads to ooiisuBQitim of liver* Allee, V. C, 1951. Cooperatlm amaag AnlTnols, vi-fch Hvinan SgpHcatloos. IfeifwwOT, Tcnrk. 233 PP* — , 1952. Dnmi nance and Merar Dominance and eiibordlnatlQQ. In the amfoHi. aogflj3h, Mustelus cauls (mtchill). Pfar^'Biol. Zool., ^{k)t3^'-3l6h. — — , and M. Doviglla, 19^5* A dotalnaace order in the hermit crab, fliygaa IfUfi^ipHDW aey. Bcology, ^tkllAlZ* Focrenen, E. M. Banks and C. H. Holabird, 1935* Sffeets of an androgen an dominance and siibordlnanco In six ccoman "breeds of Galltts ffOlus. Phyalol. Zool., 28(2)1 89-U5. B. Gbreenlwrg, a. N. BoMithal and P. f!rank, Sons effeets of social orgBnlzatioci on growth In the green simfiah, I«ponls cyanellTia. Jour» Eacp» Zool., 108:1-19. Banks, E. H., 19^6. Social orggoilzatlca ift Md twl hma [(hUxm gRilus sub^. ), Scolosy. ^i239-2.kQ, Bennett, M. A., 19^* Tbe social hlerardiy In ring donres. Ecology, 20(3)< 337-357. Bovjberg, R. V. , 1953. ilcnlnanoe co-dar In Hm en^fiah, Qronectes vlrllla Hagsn. fhyslol. Zool., SjSiXl^l^ 1956. ScDMi factors affecting a^resslre "behaivlar In cra^ah. Phyalol. Zool., ^:127-136. Biraddock, J. C, 19i^5. Soma aspects of the dnmlnanee-st&ordtnatlon relationship in the fish, Platypoecllus aaculatns. Riyslol. Zool., 2^:176-195. , 19'^9> Bie effect of prior residence vipcai danlnanee In HtM flah, Platypoecilus aaculatus. Physiol. Zool., 22:l6l-l69. Carpenter, C. B., 19l|-2. Sexual behavior of free ranging Rhesus iBODkeys (Maeaca aulatta). I. Speelmms, procedures and Isehavloral eharaeteriatloa of estrus. J. Comp. Psychol., 33;113-1^>3* ko " i eolilasj Ifi 1., 19^3. Statltstleal aaalywls of f&ctors -Hfalch make for sixccew in Intitlal «icount«r8 Txitween hens. Aa« Hat»« 21: 519-5 38« — 19Mt^. AggresBlve behavior amoog vertebrate animals. Phyalol* Zool«> 1X:83-123. Crawford, M. P., 19^^0. Tbe relation between social doalnance and the BMnstrual eyele tn fesoule chl^panzeea. J. Ckanp* Peyebol*, 30:1*83-513. 1911^2* DcBdnanee and social behavior, for chimpanzees, in a nm competative situation. J. Cflpp. Psychol., 321:267-277. Darling, F, P., I938. Bird Flocks and the Breeains Cycle. 2he Iftilverslty Press, Cafflbridge. 12^4- pp. Daudln, F. M., I803. Historie Ifaturelle des Rainettes. des Grenouilles et d6S Crapauda. Paris, p. Q5» Davies, J. L., 1953. Colony size and rao?roduction in the grey seal. Piroo. Zool. 80c. Londfln., 123(ll): 327-332. Delaney, M. J., 1955. ^ erolution of aggressive behavior in vertebrate animals (tMpiibllahed). Dept. of Zool., Glasgov, Scotland. (How at Ifetiv.of Southharaptcn, England). Dlebschlag, E., 19^1. Psychological observations on the rank order in the domestic pigeon. From the Zool. Inst, of the Ifelv. of Marburg. (PsychologLsche beobachtungsn uber die xtuigrardnimg bei der haiistaube. Zeitachr. f. Tlerpsychol.^ 4:(H.2) 173-188). DougLis, M., 191*6. Seme evidence of a flomlnnnce -subordinanee relationship onang lobsters. Anat. Rec., 2i'553» (Abstract). — — , 191*8. Social factors inflvienclng the hierarchies of aoall flx>ck8 of dOB«stic hen: lha interaction between residtat and part time loembers of or^tfiized flocks. Physiol. Zool., a:ll*7-l8a« Edwards, A. L., 1951*. Statistical Methods for the Behavioral Sciences. Rhinehart and Coorpany, Inc., Binr York. 5^*2 pp. Bllan, E., and R. W. Murray, 1951. New laterlal line sensory organs in Xenopus laevis. Nature, 168(1*272:1*77* Ck>in, Coleman J., 1953. Parsonal Conanunication. -^''^ Oola, Olive B., 1955 . World Outside >ty Poor. MacMlllaa Co., New York. p. 1*3. Gordon, K., 19l*0. Territorial behavior and social dooinance asiong Sculridae. J. Mwwaal.. 17:171-172» . . Grant, W. 1955 • •Mtrrltorlallam in two species of salaajaad^s. Science, 1S:(33.35)J137» Oreenberg, B., 191*7. Some relations between territory, social hierarchy and leadership in the greeri suailsli, Lepotnls eyan^OJLus* Riysiol. Zool« ^: 267-299. — — , and G. K. Koible, IS^th, Social behavior of the Aa»rloan chameleon, Anolis carol jnensls Volgb. Physiol. Zool». 17:-^2-4^, Oi&l, A. M., and ¥. C. Allee, 19hh, Seme measureable effects of social organization In nocks of hens, fhyglol. Zool.. 18:320-31*7. BsnMcrstroni, F., 191^2. Sondnanee in vlnter flocks of chickadees. Wilscn Bull., 54:32-1*2. Hasklns, CP., 1951 • Of Societies and Mai. Horton, ITev York. 2^ pp. Edward, W. E., and J. T. Salem, Jr., 19l»2. Intercovey social relationships in the valley quail. VUson BuU., §4:162-170. Kslth, A., 191*9. A .Tfev !Iheory of Hianan Eyolutlon. Philosophical Ubrary, New York, 1*51 pp. • ■ ; : ;.. - . - lAslle, J. M., 1890. Notes on the habits and oviposition of Xaiopus laevis« Froc. Zool. Soc.. London. 69-7I. lorenz, K., 191*1. V«*glelohttide bewegungsstudlen an anatinon'. Jour. f«' Oroithol.. 82(Pestschrlft Belnroth):19l*-29l*, ——-1 1950. The conparative method in studying innate behavior patterns. Physiological Mechanisms in Animal Behavior. Acadsmic Press, New York. 221-268. — — , 1952. King SolcEm's Ring. Ihoinas Y. Growell (Jo., New York. 202 pp. lorn, M. S., 1956. Dominance-subordination relationships in the crawfish, CambareU.us shufeldtli. Tulane Studies in Zoology. 1*(5):1^-170. Martof, B. S., 1953' Tisrrltorlallty in the greeox fSrog, Rana clnwltj^nw. Ecology^ 2it'l56-174. Maslow, A. H., 19l*0. Doodnance qfuality and social behavior in infra -human primates. J. Soc. Psychol., 11:313-321*. Munro, A. F., 1953* ^e anmonia and urea exeretion of dlffer«it species of amphibians during develosment and metaBorphosls. Biochem. Jour.. 5l*:29-36. . i '- ' Wmmei, M. a., 1956. social "behavior and Inter^ciflc canrpttltlon In two trout species. HiyaiQl. Zool.j 22,^611-81. Odbaa, I. P., 1953. FuoaacientalB of Icology* V* »• Semders Co., Philad«lpiil*. Pardl, t., 191*8. DoBiiaance order in PolisteB VAsps. Physiol. Zool., gLtl-lS* Pirker, F. Jr., S. L. Kobins sad A, LaTreriage, 19**?. Breedijas, rearlag and care of the South African clawed frog (Xeoogjs laevls). Baa Aawr. Mat*, ja: 38-^9. PBiteraoa, Bellie P., 1951. aasal cavities of "ttbe toed Haaiplpe caryalhoi Mlr-ril) and otiber Plpidae. Proc. Zool. 80c., Loadoaa, l^t2JJ3ai-iH5. Peargon, P. G., 1955 . Population ecology of the Bpedefoot toad, Seaphiopus \ Hblbroold. (Harlan). Ecological Monoscanhs, 2^:233-207. Bose, W., 3^0. The Reptiles and Actplii'biana of Southera Africa. HMfcMr Miller Ltd., Cape Town. pp. 1-3^. Sabine, W. S., 191*9. Doainance in vinter floclcs of Juncos and tree ^perrowa. Riysiol. Zool., ^:61v-85. Sdijeiaerup-EbTae, T., 1913. Honsenes stsDBie fidrag til Honsanes peyiwlogl aatiaea, 2L»2^-276. — — * 19^» Social behavior of birdsj cited In C. A. Murchlnaon, A aandbook of Social Psyciioiogy. Worchester. Pp. 9^7-972. Sharif, M., 1956. Experimeats in ffccnxp cooflict. Scientific American, 121(5) »5^-58. l^st, F. H«, 195l». Social aegresslveneas in an aacEMbian. Sci«ice, 120(3100) »1^> Tittbergan, H., 19lt2. An ob.iectivi3tic study of the innate beba-/iour of aniaals. Series D. Bibllothica Biothcoretica. V.l pars 2. 1. J. Brill, london. pp. 39-98. 19li8. Social releasers and the experimental xaethod required for their study. Wilson Bull., 60:6-52. 1953. Social Behaviour in Animals, vith Special Beferences to Vertebrates. Methuen and Co., Ltd., Lcndoi. J<^ Wiley and Soas, Inc., liev York. 150 pp. Uhrl6h, J., 1938. The social hlerawShy In aXMno mice. Jbur. Cocg). Faychol., g5.»273-^3. MetrogravTge. BBltiaiore^ Ud^p ApriLL ^i* Ytix^e, R. NU, 1939* Social <3anlnanoe and seaenaT itatns in the nMnrpan: ghe Quarterly Beviev of Biology. l^t»115<436« BIOGSRAPHICAIi X9SM8 Bobert Bice Baubrlcli vas Ixxax In CilscraBiaat, Sew VSaxagOilxtif Hay k, 19S% aad gracbuaiwd txm St«r«u EL^ fichool^ GLaxmaeat, ija. June, 19^. H» catered the t]Dlv«r8l% ot Wn Vtupi&sILn in iSm ftiSX of 19^ and after thrott MMsters entered the Axny Air Force lAiere he sp«it three years. In ibrch^ 191^, he entered ItLchlgaa 8tate IMTer^i^ and receiwA a Bachelor of Science degree in Forestry (l9*^9) and a Master of Science degree In Zcxtlogy (19^2) £roa that inatituticn. Ihe sisimer of 19^2, iias spent at Point Barrov, ilLaska, vith the Office of Iforal BesMxtfli. He \m§m his dcxrtoral program at the IMversity of KLorlda in September, I952 and spent the ■ini II of 19^3 and 19^ at the ifarine Biological Laboratory, Mooda Hole, ilHHnchusetts. VhUe at the Oiiversity of florlda he tan£^ biology Inlxn-atory and ganeral biology (C-61, C-^} and also vas Assistant Baaiteit Adrlsor In one of the housing areas on canqpus* Haubrloh Is a neaber of XL Si0Mi fl, Rd BL^m, Blgna XI, fhe Association, foor the Study of Aoifflal Behaviour and the Ecological Society of Aaerloa* ahls dlasortatioQ "was prepared under the direction of the chairman of -aift candidate ' s supervisory committee and has "be«i approved by all msmberB of the conunittee. It was suhmitted to the Dean of the College of Arts and Sciences and to the Graduate Cornell and vaa apjaroved as partial fulfillaent of the reqvUreoi^its for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy. Au^ist 10, 1957 i-'<' "Smmf CkxUefte of Arts azid Sciences Been, Graduate School SUHSR7IS0KY