HARVARD UNIVERSITY Library of the Museum of Comparative Zoology HARVARD UNIVERSITY Library of the Museum of Comparative Zoology THE INT£KNATIOi\AL SCIENTIFIC SERIES. VOLUME XLIX. THE INTERNATIONAL SCIENTIFIC SERIES. Each book complete in One Volume, 12mo, and bound in Cloth. 1. FORMS OF WATER; A Familiar Exposition of the Orijrin aud Phenomena of Glaciers. By J. Tyndall, LL. D., F, R. S. With 25111UBtration8. $1.50. 2. PHYSICS AND POLITICS^ Or, Thoughts on the Application of the Prin- ciples of "Natural Selection" and "Inheritance'' to Political Society. By Waltek Bagehot. $1,.0. 3. FOODS. By Edwakd Smith, M. D., LL. B,, F. R. S. With numerous Illus- trations. $1.75. 4. MIND AND BODY : The Theoriop of their Relation. By Alexander Bain, LL. D. With 4 Illustrations. |1.50. 5. THE STUDY OF SOCIOLOGY. By Herbert Spencer. $1.50. 6. THE NEW CHEMISTRY. By Professor J. P. Cooke, of Harvard Uuiver- eity. With 31 Illustrations. $'^.00. 7. ON THE CONSERVATION OF ENERGY. By Balpocr Stewart, M. A., LL. D., F. R. S. With 14 Illustrations, $1.50. 8. ANIMAL LOCOMOTION; or. Walk in e, Swimming, and Flying, By J. B. Pettigrew, M. D., F. R. S., etc. With 130 Illustrations. $1.75. 9. R-lSP )NSIBILITY IN MENTAL DISEASE. By Henry Maudslet, M. D. $1.60. 10. THE SCIENCE OF LAW. By Professor Sheldon Amos. $1 75. 11. AMIMAL MECHANISM: A Treatise on Terrestrial and Aerial Locomotion, By Professor E. J. Mahet. With 117 Illustrations. i>\ 75. 12. THE HISTORY OF THE CONFLICT BETWEEN RELIGION AND SCI- ENCE. By J. W. Draper, M. D., LL. D. $1.75. :3. THE DOCTRINE OF DESCENT AND DARWINISM. By Professor Oscar Schmidt (Sirasburg University). With 26 Illustrations. $1.50. 14. THE CHEMICAL EFFECTS OF LIGHT AND PHOTOGRAPHY. By Dr. Hermann Vogel (Polytechnic Academy of Berlin\ Translation thoroughly revised. Wifji 100 Illustrations. SS.OO. 15. FUNGI: Their Nature, Influences. Uses, etc. By M. C. Cooke, M. A., LL. D. Edited by the ilev. M. J. Berkeley, M. A., F.L.S. With 109 Illustrations. $1.50. 16. THE LIFE AND GROWTH OF LANGUAGE. By Professor William Dwight WHffNEY, of Yale Collfge. $1.50. New Toi-k . D. APPLETON & CO., 1, 3, & 5 Bond Street, The International Scientific 5erie«— (Continued.) 17. MONEY AND THE MECHANISM OF EXCHANGE. By W. Stanley Jevons, M.A., F.R. S. !?1.T5. IS. THE NATURE OF LIGHT, with a General .\ccount of Physical Opiicf. By Dr. EuGKNE LoMMKL. With 1S8 lUus't rations and a Table of Spectra iu l'hromo-liilio;;raphy. $2.00. 19. ANIMAL PAR.XSITES AND MESSMATKS. By Monsieur Van Benkuen. With 8;i Illustrations. *1.50. 20. FERMENTATION. By TrofeBSor SchCtzenbekger. With 2s Illustrations. j;l.:0. 21. THE FIVE SENSES OF MAN. By Professcr Eeknstein. With 91 lUus- tratioi.8. §1.75. 22. THE THEORY OF SOUND IN ITS RELATION TO MUSIC. By Pro- fcsiror PiETKO Blaserna. With numerous Illustratioiis. %\.hQ. 23. STUDIES IN SPECTRUM ANALYSIS. By J. Nokman Lockter, F. R. S. With 6 Pii((to;;r!iphic Illustrations cf Spectra, and numerous Engravinjrs on Wood. |;2.."0. 24. A HISTORY OF THE GROWTH OF THE STEAM-ENGINE. By Pro- fisi^cr R. 11. Thurston. With 163 Illusirations. $2.50. 25. EDUCATION AS A SJlENCE. By Alexander B/ in, LL.D. $1.75. 26. STUDENTS' TEXT-BOOK ( F COLOR; Or, Modem Clircmatics. WMth .-^'Plications to Art find Industry. By Piolcssor Ogden N. Rood, Colum- bia College. New edition. Witii 130 Illustrations. $2.00. 27. TH'v HUMAN SPECIES. By Professor A be Quateefages, Membrc de 1 [n^titut. $2.C0. 28. THF, CRAYFI-sH: An Introduction to thp Study of ZooloL'y- By T. H. HuxLET, F. R.S, W^ith £2 Illustrations, fl.75. 29. THE ATOMIC THEORY. By Professor A. Wur.Tz. Translated by E. Clcniinshaw, F. C. S. $1.50. 80. ANIMAL LIFE AS AFFECTED BY THE NATURAL CONDITIONS OF EXP^TENCE, By Karl Semper. Wiih 2 Maps and 106 Woodcuts. IIXQ. SI. SIGHT: .\n Exposition of ihe Prinriplop of Mcnocnl'ir and Binocular Vition. By Joseph Le Co:jte, LL. D. With 132 Illustrations. ^,1.50. 82. GENi-RAL I'HYSIOTOGY OF MUSCLES AND NERVES. By Professor J. Ropf.NTOAL. With 75 Illustrations, f.1.50. 83. ILLUSIONS: A Psycho'o^ical Study. P.y James Sullv. $1.50. 34. THE SUN. By C. A. Youno. Professor of Astronomy in the College of New Jersey. With numerous Illusirations. $2.00. New York : D. APPLETON & CO., 1, 3, &, 5 Bond Street. Tfie Inttrnatiunal iicientijic -i!e7i6«-i,Coutiuued.) 55. VOLCANOES: What they Are aucl what they Teach. Ey Juu.n W. Judd, F. R. S., Profet^bor of Ucolugy m Uie Itoval Sciaooi ol' Miutt. WuhUtill- lustralious. %-i, 00. 36 SUICIDE: Au Es.^ay in Coaiparative Moral Statistics. By Henuy Mor- SKLLi M.D., Prokssor ol IbycLoio^ical fiieaieiije, Rcyal Lnivcibily, Turin. $1.75. il Tim FORMATION OF VEGli-TABLE SIOLLD, THROUGH TUK AC- TION OF VVOUMS. With ObsLrvalious on thcii Habits. By CHARiJia Darwin, LL. D., F. R. S. With lUubtraiioub. $1.50. 38. THE CONCEPTS AND THEORIES OF MODERN PHYSICS. By J. B. Staxlo. $1.75. .59. THE BHAIN AND ITS FUNCTIONS. By J. LiYS. $1 £0. 40. MYTH AND SCIENCE. By Tito Vignoli. $150. il. DISEASES OF MRMORY: An Essay iu the Positive Psychology. By Th. RiBOT, author of " Heredity." $1.50. 4-2. ANTS, BEES. AND WASPS. A Record of Observations of the Fabits of the Social Hymeiioptera. By Sir John Lubbock, Bart., F. R. S-, D. C. L., LL. D., etc. $-2.09. 43. SCIENCE OF POLITICS. By Sheldon Amos, f 1.75. 44. ANI.MAL INTELLIGENCE. By George J. Romanes. $1.75. 45. MAN BEFORE METALS. By N. Jolt, Corrcspordcnt of tie Institute, With 148 Illustrations, gl.75. 46. THE ORGANS OF SPSE^H .AND THFIR AFPLICATIGN IN THE FORMATION OF ARTICULATE SOUNDS, By G. H von Metet?, Pro- fessor in Ordinary of Anatomy at the University of Ziirich. With 47 Woodcuts. ^1.75. 47. FALLACIES: A View of Logic from the Practical Side. By Alfred SiDGWICK, B.A., OXOD. $1.75. 48. ORIGIN OF CULTIVATED PLANTS. By Alphonse ue Caneolle. §2.00. 49. JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. Beinc: a Research on Primitive Nervous Systems. By Geobge J. Romanes. $1 75. 50. THE COMMON SENSE OP THE EXACT SCIENCES. By tli;^ late Will- L&.M KiNGDON Clifford, «"! 50. 51 PHYSICAL EXPRESSION: Its Modes and Principles. By Francis War^ • NER, M. D., Assistant Physician, and Lertnrer on Botany to the London Hospital, etc. With 51 Ilhistrations. SI, 75. 32. ANTHROPOID APES. By Robert Hartmann, Professor in the University of Berlin. With 63 Illustrations. $1.75. New York : D. APPLETON & CO., 1, 3, & 5 Boni Street, The Jntemaliofial Scientific Stiies.— (Continued.) 53. THE 3IAMMALIA IN THEIR RELATION TO PRIMEVAL TIMES. By Oscar Schmidt. §1.50. 54. COMPARATIVE LITERATURE. By Hutcheson Macaulat Posnett, M. A., LL. D., F. L. S., Earrister-at-Law ; Professor of Classics and English Litciaturo, University College, Auckland, New Zealand, author of "The Historical Method," etc. *1.75. 55. EARTHQUAKES AND OTHER EARTH MOVE^ ENTS. By John Milne, Professor of Mining and Geology in the Imperial College of Engineering, Tokio, Japan. With 38 Figures. $1.75. 50. MICROBES, FERMENTS, AND MOULDS. By E. L. Tuouessakt. With 107 Illastiations. §1.50. 57. THE GEOGRAPHICAL AND GEOLOGICAL DISTRIBUTION OF ANI- MALS. By Angelo Heilprin. $2.00. 58. WEATHER. A Popular Expot ition of the Nature of Weather Changes from Day to Day. With Diagrams. By Hon. Ralph ABERCRoaiBY. $1.75. 59. ANIMAL MAGNETISM. By Alfred Binet and Charles Fef.e, Assistant Physician at the Salpetriere. %,1.1Q. 60. INTERNATIONAIi LAW, with Materials for a Code of International Law. By Leone Levi, Professor of Common Law, King's College, $1.50. 61. THE GEOLOGICAL HISTORY OF PLANTS. WHth Illnstratione. By Sir J. WiiJJtAM Dawson, LL.D., F.R. S. $1.75. 62. ANTHROPOLOGY. An Introduction to the Study of Man and Civilixation. By Edward B. Tylor, D. C. L., F. R. S. Illustrated. $2.00. 63. THE ORIGIN OF FLORAL STRUCTURES, THROUGH INSECT AND OTHER AGENCIES. By the Rev. George Henslow, M. A., F. L. S., F.G. S. With 88 Illustrations. $1.75. 04. ON THE SENSES, INSTINCTS, AND INTELLIGENCE OF ANIMALS, WITH SPECIAL REFERENCE TO INSECTS. By Sir John Lvbbock. With over 100 Illustrations. $1.75. 65. THE PRIMITIVE FAMILY IN ITS ORIGIN AND DEVELOPMENT. By Dr. C. N. Starcke, of the University of Copenhagen. $1.75. 66. PHYSIOLOGY OF BODILY EXERCISE. By Fernand Lagrange, M.D. $1.75. 67. THE COLOURS OF ANIMALS: Their Meaning and Use. By Edward Bagsall PouLTON, F. R. S. $1.75. 68. SOCIALISM : New and Old. By William Graham, M. A., Professor of < Political Economy and Jurisprudence, Queen's College, Belfast. $1.75. 69. MAN AND THE GLACIAL PERIOD. By G. Frederick Wright, D. D., LL. D., F. G. S. A., Professor in Oberlin Theological Seminary ; Assistant on the U. S. Geological Survey. With Maps and Illustrations. THE INTERNATIONAL SCIENTIPIC SERIES. JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH AND SEA-URCHINS BEING A RESEARCH ON PRIMITIVE NERVOUS SYSTEMS BY G. J. ROMANES, 31. A., LL. D., F. R. S. ZOOLOGICAL SECEETAEY OF THK LISXEAX SOCIETT NEW YORK: D. APPLETOX AND COMPANY, 1, 3, AND 5 BO^'D STREET. 1893. ■J ■' ^^; L/BRARV DEC 2 2 1999 UNfVERsjTv BIOLOGICAL LABORATORIES LIBRAITI hlAfty/\RD UNIVERSITY PREFACE. When I first accepted the invitation of the editors of the International Scientific Series to supply a book upon Primitive Nervous Systems, I intended to have supplemented the description of my own work on the physiology of the Medusce and Echinodermata with a tolerably full exposition of the results which have been obtained by other inquirers concerning the morphology and develop- ment of these animals. But it soon became apparent that it would be impossible, within the limits assigned to me, to do justice to the more important investigations upon these matters ; and therefore I eventually decided upon restricting this essay to an account of my own researches. With the exception of a few Avoodcuts in the last chapter (for the loan of which I am indebted to the kindness of Messrs. Cassell), all the illustra- tions are either original or copies of those in my Royal Society papers. In the letter-press also I have not scrupled to draw upon these papers. X PREFACE. wlierever it seemed to me that the passages would })e suihciently intelligible to a general reader. I may observe, however, that although I ha\-o throughout kept in view the requirements of a general reader, I have also sought to render the book of service to the working physiologist, by brinsfinix tooether in one consecuti\ e account all the more important observations and results which liave been yielded by this research. G. J. R. IX)NDON, 1884. CONTENTS, CHAPTKR PACK Introduction ... ,„ ... „, ,„ 1 I. Structure of the RlEDusiE ,,, .., 10 II. Fundamental Experiments ... ... ... 26 III. Experiments in Stimulation ... ... 87 IV. Experiments in Section of Covered-Eyed ^Iedus^ 05 V. Experiments in Section of Naked-Eyed INIedus^ 104 VI. Co-ordination... ... ... ... ... i;>0 VII. Natural Rhythm ... ,,, ,„ ... 14G VIH- Artificial Rhythm ... ... „, ... 175 IX. Poisons ... ... ... .„ ... 218 X. Star-Fish and Ska-Ueciiins ,„ „, ... 251 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-UECHLNS. INTRODUCTION. Among the most beautiful, as well as the most common, of the marine animals which are to be met Avith upon our coasts are the jelly-fish and the star- fish. Scarcely any one is so devoid of the instincts either of the artist or of tlie naturalist as not to have watched these animals with blended emotions of the ?esthctic and the scientific — feeling the beauty while wondering at the organization. How many of us who live for most of the year in the fog and dust of large towns enjoy with the greater zest our summer's holiday at the seaside ? And in the memories of most of us is there not associated with the picture of breaking waves and sea-birds floating indiffeiently in the blue sky or on the water still more blue, the thoughts of many a ramble among the weedy rocks and living pools, where for the time being we all become naturalists, and where those who least know what they are likely to find 2 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. in tlieir search are most Lkely to approach the keen happiness of cliiklhood ? If so, the image of the red sea-stars bespangling a mile of shining sand, or decorating the darkness of a thousand grottoes, must be joined with the image, no less vivid, of those crystal globes pulsating with life and gleam- ino- with all the colours of the rainbow, which are perhaps the most strange, and certainly in my estimation the most delicately lovely creatures in the world. It is with these two kinds of creatures that the present work is concerned, and if it seems almost impious to lay the " forced fingers rude " of science upon living things of such exquisite beauty, let it be remembered that our human nature is not so much out of joint that the rational desire to know is incompatible with the emotional impulse to admire. Speaking for myself, I can testify that my admiration of the extreme beauty of these animals has been greatly enhanced —or rather I should say that this extreme beauty has been, so to speak,- revealed — by the continuous and close observation which many of my experiments required : both with the unassisted eye and with the mici'oscope numberless points of detail, unnoticed before, became familiar to the mind; the forms as a whole were impressed upon the memory; and, by constantly watching their movements and changes of appear- ance, I have grown, like an artist studying a face or a landscape, to appreciate a fulness of beauty, the esse of which is only rendered possible by the pc9' ci'pi of such attention as is demanded by scientiiic INTRODUCTION. 3 research. Moreover, association, if not the sole creator, is at least a most important factor of the beautiful; and therefore the sight of one ol these animals is now much more to me, in the respects which we are considering, than it can be to any one in whose memory it is not connected with many days of that purest form of enjoy- ment which cfin only be experienced in the pursuit of science. And here I may observe that the worker in marine zoology has one great advantage over his other scientific brethren. Apart from the intrinsic boaut}^ of most of the creatures with which he has to deal, all the accompaniments of his work are resthetic, and removed from those more or less offensive features which cire so often necessaril}* incidental to the study of anatomy and physiolog}' in the higher animals. When, for instance, I con- trast my oAvn work in a town laboratory on vertebrated animals with that which I am now about to describe upon the invertebrated in a laboratory set up upon the sea-beach, it is im- possible not to feel that the contrast in point of enjoyment is considerable. In the latter case, a summer's work resembles the pleasure-making of a picnic prolonged for months, with the sense of feel-, ing all the while that no time is being profitlessly spent. Whether one is sailing about upon the sunny sea, fishing with muslin nets for the surface fauna, or steaming away far from shore to dredge for othermaterial, or, again, carrying on observations in the cool sea-water tanks and bell-jars of a neat ^ JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. little wooJen worksliop thrown open to the sea- breezes, it alike requires some effort to persuade one's self that the occupation is really something more than that of finding amusement. It is now twelve years since I first took to this kind of summer recreation, and during that time most of my attention while at the seaside has been devoted to the two classes of animals already men- tioned— viz. the jelly-fish and star-fish, or, as naturalists have named them, the Medusie and Echinodermata. The present volume contains a tolerably full account of the results which during six of these summers I have succeeded in obtaining. If any of my readers should thiidc that the harvest appears to be a small one in relation to the time and labour spent in gathering it, I shall feel prett}^ confident that those readers are not themselves working physiologists, and, therefore, that they are reall}^ ignorant of the time and labovu' required to devuse and execute even apparently simple experi- ments, to hunt down a physiological question to its only possible answer, and to verify each step in the process of an experimental proof. Moreover, the difficulties in all these respects are increased tenfold in a seaside laboratory without adequate equipments or attendance, and where, in conse- quence, more time is usually lost in devising make- shifts for apparatus, and teaching unskilled hands how to help, than is consumed in all other parts of a research. From the picnic point of view, how- ever, there is no real loss in this; such incidental difficulties add to the enjoyment (else why choose to INTRODUCTION. 5 make an extemporized grate and boil a kettle in the wood, when a much more efficient grate, full of lighted coals, is already boiling some other kettle at home?); and if they somewhat unduly prolong a research, the full meaning of life is, after all, not exhausted by the experiences of a mill-horse, and it is well to remem- ber that so soon as we cease to take pleasure in our work, we are most likely sacrificing one part of our humanity to the altar of some other, and probably less worthy, constituent. I may now say a few words on the scope of the investigations which are to be described in the present treatise. To some extent this is conveyed by the title ; but I may observe that, as the " primi- tive nervous systems" whose physiology I have sought to advance are mainly subservient to the office of locomotion, in my Royal Society papers upon these researches I have adopted the title of '' Observations on the Locomotor System " of each of the classes of animals in question. It is of interest to notice in this connection that the plan or mechanism of locomotion is completely different in the two classes, and that in the case of each class the plan or mechanism is unique, i.e. is not to be met with elsewhere in the animal kingdom. It is curious, however, that, in the case of one family of star-fish (the Comatulce), owing to an extreme modification of form and function presented by the constituent parts of the locomotor organs, the method of progression has come closely to resemble that which is characteristic of jelly-fish. There is still one preliminary topic on which I 6 JELLY-FISH, STAE-FISII, AND SEA-URCHINS. feel tliat it is dcsivcable to toucli before proceeding to give au account of my experiments, and this has reference to the vivisection which many of t])cse experiments have entailed. But in saying what I have to say in this connection I can afford to be brief, inasmuch as it is not needful to discuss tho so-called vivisection question. I have merely to make it plain that, so far as the experiments which I am about to describe are concerned, there is not an}^ reasonable ground for supposing that pain can have been suffered by the animals. And this it is easy to show ; for the animals in question are so low in the scale of life, that to suppose them capable of conscious suffering would be in the higliest degree unreasonable. Thus, for instance, they are considerably lower in the scale of organization tlian an oyster, and in none of the experiments which I have performed upon them has so much laceration of Jivino' tissue been entailed as that whicli is caused by opening an oyster and eating it alive, after due application of pepper and vinegar. There- fore, if any one should be foolish enough to object to my experiments on the score of vivisection, a fortiori they are bound to object to the culinary use of oysters. Of course, it may be answered to this that two blacks do not make a white, and that I have not by this illustration succeeded in proving my negative. To this, however, I may in turn reply that, for the purpose of morally justifying my experiments on the ground which I have adopted, it is not incumbent on me to prove any negative ; it is rather for my critics to prove a positive. That INTRODUCTION. 7 is to say, before convincing me of sin, it must be shown that there is some reasonable ground for supposing that a jelly-ti.sh or a star-fish is capable of feeling pain. I submit that there is no such ground. Tlie mere fact that the animals are alive constitutes no sucli ground ; for the insectivorous plants are also alive, and exhibit even more phy- siological " sensitiveness " and capability of rapid response to stimulation than is the case with the animals which we are about to consider. And if anyone should go so far as to object to Mr. Darwin's experiments on these plants on account of its not being demonstrable that the tissues did not suffer under his operations, such a person is logically bound to go still further, and to object on similar grounds to the horrible cruelty of skinning potatoes and boilino* them alive. o « Thus, before any rational scruples can arise with regard to the vivisection of a living organism, some reasonable ground must be shown for supposing that the organism, besides being living, is also capable of suffering. But no such reasonable ground can be shown in the case of these low animals. We only know of such capability in any case through the analogy based upon our own experi- ence, and, if we trust to this analogy, we must con- clude that the capability in question vanishes long before we come to animals so low in the scale as the jelly-fish or star-fish. For within the limits of our own organism we have direct evidence that nervous mechanisms, much more highly elaborated than any of those which we are about to consider, 8 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. are incapable of suffering. Thus, for instance, when the nervous continuity of the spinal cord is inter- rupted, so that a stimulus applied to the lower extremities is unable to pass upwards to the brain, the feet will be actively drawn away from a source of irritation without the man being conscious of any pain ; the loAver nervous centres in the spinal cord respond to the stimulation, but they do so without feeling the stimulus. In order to feel there must be consciousness, and, so far as our evidence goes, it appears that consciousness only arises when a nerve-centre attains to some such degree of complexity and elaboration as are to be met with in the brain. Whether or not there is a dawning consciousness in any nerve-centres con- siderably lower in the scale of nervous evolution, is a question which we cannot answer; but we may be quite certain that, if such is the case, the consciousness which is present must be of a com- mensurately dim and unsuffering kind. Conse- quently, even on this positive aspect of the question, we may be quite sure that by the time we come to the jelly-fish — where the object of the experiments in the first instance was to obtain evidence of the very existence of nerve-tissue — all question of pain must have vanished. Whatever opinions, therefore, we may severally entertain on the vexed question of vivisection as a whole, and with whatever feel- ings we may regard the " blind Fury " who, in the person of the modern physiologist, "comes with the abhorred shears and slits the thin-spun life," INTRODUCTION. 9 we should be all agreed that in the case of these animals the life is so very thin-spun that any suggestion of abhorrence is on the face of it absurd.* * The relfition of consciousness to the elaboration of nerve- centres throughout the animal kingdom is more fully considered in my work on "Mental Evolution in Animals" (Kcgan Paul, Trench & Co. : 1SS3). CPJ AFTER I. STRUCTURE OF THE MEDUSA To give a full account of the morphology, develop- ment, and classification of the MedusiB would be both unnecessary for our present purposes and impracticable within the space which is allotted to the present work.* But, for the sake of clearness in what follows, I shall begin by briefly describing such features in the anatomy of the jelly-fish as will afterwards be found especially to concern us. In size, the different species of Medusa? vary from that of a small pea to that of a large umbrella having streamers a hundred feet long. The general form of these animals varies in different species from that of a thimble (Fig. 1) to that of a bowl, a parasol, or a saucer (see figures in subsequent chap- ters). Or we may say that the form of the animals always resembles that of a mushroom, and that the resemblance * Those who may desire to read an excellent epitome of our most recent knowledge on these subjects, may refer to Professor E. Ray Lankestei's article in the '* Encyclopaedia Britauuica " on '* Hydrozoa," together with Professor Haeckel's Report on the Medusae of the ChaZlenger Expedition. STEUCTUrvE OF THE MEDUSA. 11 extends to a tolerably close imitation by different species of the various forms which are characteristic of di^^erent species of mushrooms, from the thimble- like kinds to the saucer-like kinds. Moreover, this accidental resemblance to a nmshroom is in- creased by the presence of a central organ, occupy- ing the position of, and more or less resembling in form, the stalk of a mushroom. This or£ran is called the " manubrium," on account of its looking like the "handle" of an umbrella, and the term " umbrella " is applied to the other portion of the animal. The manubrium, like the umbrella, varies much in size and shape in different species, as a glance at any figures of these animals will show. Both the manubrium and umbrella are almost entirely composed of a thick, transparent, and non- contractile jelly ; but the whole surface of the manubrium and the whole concave surface of the umbrella are overlayed by a thin layer or sheet of contractile tissue. This tissue constitutes the earliest appearance in the animal kingdom of true muscular fibres, and its thickness, Avhich is pretty uniform, is nowhere greater than tliat of very thin paper. The manubrium is the mouth and stomach of the animal, and at the point where it is attached to or suspended from the umbrella its central cavity opens into a tube-system, which radiates through the lower or concave aspect of the umbrella. This tube-system, which serves to convey digested ma- terial and may therefore be regarded as intestinal in function, presents two different forms in the two 12 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. main groups into which the Medust'B are divided. In the "naked-eyed" group, the tubes are un- branched and run in a straight course to the margin of the umbrella, where they open into a common circular tube which runs all the way round the margin (see Figs. 1 and 22). In the " covered-eyed " group, on the other hand, the tubes are strongly branched (see Fig. 8), although they likewise all eventually terminate in a single circular tube. This circular or marginal tube in both cases communi- cates by minute apertures with the external medium. The margin of the umbrella, both in the naked and covered eyed Medusre, supports a series of con- tractile tentacles, w^hich vary greatly in size and number in different species (see Figs. 1 and 8). The margin also supports another series of bodies which will presently be found to be of much importance for us. These are the so-called " marginal bodies," w^hich vary in number, size, and structure in different species. In all the covered- e^^ed species these marginal bodies occur in the form of little bags of crystals (therefore they are called " litho- cysts "), which are protected by curiously formed "hoods " or " covers " of gelatinous tissue ; and it is on this account that the group is called " covered- eyed," in contradistinction to the "naked-eyed," where these little hoods or coverings are invariably absent (compare Fig. 1 with Fig. 22), and the crystals frequently so. In nearly all cases these marginal bodies contain more or less brightly coloured pigments. The question whether any nervous tissue is STRUCTURE OF THE MEDUSiE. 18 present in the Medusse is one which has long occupied tlie more or less arduous labours of many naturalists. The question attracted so much in- vestigation on account of its being one of unusual interest in biology. Nerve-tissue had been clearly shown to occur in all animals higher in the zoolo- gical scale than the Medusae, so that it was of much importance to ascertain whether or not the first occurrence of this tissue was to be met with in this class. But, notwithstanding the diligent appli- cation of so much skilled labour, up to the time when my own researches began there had been so little agreement in the results obtained by the numerous investigators, that Professor Huxley — himself one of the greatest authorities upon the group — thus defined the position of the matter in his " Classification of Animals " (p. 22) : " No nervous system has yet been discovered in any of these animals." The following is a list of the more impor- tant researches on this topic up to the time which I have just named : — Ehrenberg, " Die Acalephen des rothen Meeres und der Organismvs der Medusen der Ostsee," Berlin, 1836 ; Kolliker, " Ueber die Randkorper der Quallen, Polypen und Strahlthiere," Froriep's neue Notizen, bd. xxv., 1843; Von Beneden, "Memoire sur les Campanu- laires de la cote d'Ostende," " Memoires de r Academic de Bruxelles," vol. xvii., 1843; Desor, "Sur la Generation Medusipare des Polypes hydraires," "Annales d. Scienc. Natur. ZooL," ser. iii. t. xii. p. 204 ; Krohn, '' Ueber Podocoryna carnea," 14 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-UIICHINS. '• Archiv. f. Naturgesclnclite," 1851, b. i. ; McCrady, *■' Descriptions of Oceania, etc.," " Proceedings of the Elliot Society of Natural History," vol. i., 1S59 ; L. Agassiz, " Contributions to the Acalipha) of North America," "J^Iemoirs of the American Academy of Arts and Sciences," vol. iii., 18G0, vol. iv., 18G2; Leuckart, "Archiv. f. Naturge- schichte," Jahrg. 38, b. ii., 1872 ; Hensen, " Studien liber das Gehororgan der Decapoden," " Zeitchr. 1 wiss. ZooL," bd. xiii., 1863 ; Semper, ** Reisebcricht," " Zeitschr. f. wiss. ZooL," bd. xiii. vol. xiv. ; Glaus, *' Bemerknngen iiber Glenophoren nnd Medusen," "Zeitschr. f. wiss. ZooL," bd. xiv., 18G4 ; Allman, "Note on the Structure of Gertain H3^droid MeduscV "Brit. Assoc. Rep.," 1807; Fritz Muller, " Polypen und Quallen von S. Gatherina," " Archiv. f. Naturgesch.," Jahrg. 25, bd. i., 1859; also "Ueber die Rand-blaschen der Hydroidquallen," " Archiv. f. Anatomic und Physiologic," 1852 ; Haeckel, "Beitrage zur Naturgesch. der Hydromedusen," 1865; Eimer, "Zoologische Untersuchungen," Wurz- burg, " Verhandlungen der Phys.-med. Gesellschaft," N.F. vL bd., 1874^. The most important of these memoirs for us to consider are the two last. I shall subsequently consider the work of Dr. Eimer, which up to this date was of a purely physiological character. Pro- fessor Haeckel, who made his microscopical obser- vations chiefly upon the Geryonidro, desciibed the nervous elements as forming a continuous circle all round the margin of the umbrella, following the course of the radial or nutrient tubes throughout STRUCTURE OF THE MEDUSA. lo their entire length, and proceeding also to the ten- tacles and marginal bodies. At the base of each tentacle there is a ganglionic swelling, and it is IVoni these ganglionic swellings that the nerves just mentioned take their origin. The most conspicuous of these nerves are those that proceed to the radial canals and marginal bodies, wdiile the least con- spicuous are those that proceed to the tentacles. Cells, as a rule, can only be observed in the gan- glionic swellings, where they appear as fusiform and distinctly nucleated bodies of great transparency and high refractive power. On the other hand, the nerves that emanate from the ganglia are composed of a delicate and transparent tissue, in which no cellular elements can be distinguished, but which is longitudinally striated in a manner very suggestive of fibrillation. Treatment with acetic acid, how- ever, brings out distinct nuclei in the case of the nerves that are situated in the marc^inal vesicles, while in those that accompany the radial canals ganglion-cells are sometimes met with. A brief sketch of the contents of these and other memoirs on the histology of the Medusfe is given by Drs. Hertwig in their more recently published work on the nervous system and sense-organs of the Medusae, and these authors point to the important fact that before the appearance of Haeckel's memoir, Leuckart was the only observer who spoke for the fibrillar character of the so-called marojinal rino- nerve ; so that in Haeckel's researches on Geryonia, whereby both true ganglion -cells and true nerve- fibres were first demonstrated as occurring in the If) JELLY-FISH, Medus?e, we have a most important step in. the histology of these animals. Haeckel's results in these respects have since been confirmed by Clans, "Grundziige der Zoologie," 1872; Allman, "A Monograph of the Gymnoblastic or Tabularian Hydroids," 1871 ; Harting, " Notices Zoologiques," Niedlandisches "Archiv. f ZooL," bd. ii., Heft 3, 1873 ; F. E. Schulze, " Ueber den Ban von Syncorzne Sarsii " ; 0. and R. Hertwig, " Das Nervensystem und die Sinnesorgane der Medusen." The last-named monograph is much the most important that has appeared upon the histology of the Medusa?. I shall, therefore, give a condensed epitome of the leading results which it has estab- lished. There is so great a difference between the nervous system of the naked and of the covered eyed Medusse, that a simultaneous description of the nervous system in both groups is not by these authors considered practicable. Beginning, there- fore, with the naked-eyed division, they describe the nervous system as here consisting of two parts, a central and a peripheral. The central part is localized in the margin of the swimming-bell, and there forms a " nerve-ring," which is divided by the insertion of the " veil " * into an upper and a lower nerve-ring. In many species the upper nerve-ring is spread out in the form of a flattish layer, which * This is the name given to a small annular sheet of tissue which forms a kind of floor to the orifice of the swimming-bell, through the central opening of which floor the manubrium passes, The structure is shown in Fig. 1. STKUCTUHE OF THE MEDUSAE. 17 is somewhat thickened where it is in contact with the veil. In these species the nerve-ring is only indistinctly marked off' from the surrounding tissues. But in other species the crowding together of the nerve-fibres at the insertion of the veil gives rise to a considerable concentration of nervous structures ; while in others, again, this concentration proceeds to the extent of causing a well-defined swelling of nervous tissue against the epithelium of the veil and umbrella. In the Geryonidse this swelling is still further strengthened by a peculiar modification of the other tissues in the neighbourhood, which had been previously described by Professor Haeckel. In all species the upper nerve-ring lies entirely in the ectoderm. Its principal mass is composed of nerve- fibres of wonderful tenuity, among which are to be found sparsely scattered ganglion-cells. The latter are for the most part bi-polar, more seldom multi- polar. The fibres which emanate from them are very delicate, and, becoming mixed with others, do not admit of being further traced. Where the nervous tissue meets the enveloping epithelium it is connected with the latter from within, but diflTers widely from it ; for the nerve-cells contain a longi- tudinally striated cjdindrical or thread-like nucleus which carries on its peripheral end a delicate hair, while its central end is prolonged into a fine nerve- fibre. There are, besides these, two other kinds of cells which form a transition between the ganglion and the epithelium cells. The first kind are of a long and cylindrical form, the free ends of which reach as far as the upper surface of the epithelium. 18 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. The second kind lie for the most part under the upper surface. They are of a large size, and pre- sent, coursing towards the upper surface, a long continuation, which at its free extremity supports a hair. In some cases this continuation is smaller, and stops short before reaching the outer surface. Drs. Hertwig observe that in these peculiar cells we have tissue elements which become more and more like the ordinary ganglion-cells of the nerve-ring the more that their lona: continuation towards the surface epithelium is shortened or lost, and these authors are thus led to conclude that the upper nerve-ring was originally constituted only by such prolongations of the epithelium-cells, and that after- wards these prolongations gradually disappeared, leaving only their remnants to develop into the ordinary ganglion-cells already described. Beneath the upper nerve-ring lies the lower nerve-ring. It is inserted between the muscle- tissue of the veil and umbrella, in the midst of a broad strand wherein muscle-fibres are entirely absent. It here constitutes a thin though broad layer which, like the upper nerve-ring, belongs to the ectoderm. It also consists of the same elements as the upper nerve-ring, viz. of nerve-fibres and ganglion-cells. Yet there is so distinct a difference of character between the elements composing the two nerve-rings, that even in an isolated portion it is easy to tell from which ring the portion has been taken. That is to say, in the lower nerve- ring there are numerous nerve-fibres of considerable thickness, which contrast in a striking manner with STRUCTUKE OF THE MEDUSA. 19 the almost immeasurably slender fibres of the upper nerve-ring. A second point of difference consists in the surprising Avcalth of ganglion-cells in the one ring as compared witli the other. Thus, on the whole, there is no doubt that the lower nerve-ring pi-esents a higher grade of structure than does the upper, as shown not only by the greater multiplicity of nerve-cells and fibres, but also by the relation in which these elements stand to the epithelium. For in the case of the lower nerve- ring, the presumably primitive connections of the nervous elements with the epithelium is w^ell-nigh dissolved — this nerve-ring having thus separated itself from its parent structure, and formed for itself an independent layer beneath the epithelium. The two nerve-rings are separated from one another by a very thin membrane, which, in some species at all events, is bored through by strands of nerve- fibres which serve to connect the two nerve-rings with one another. The pei'ipheral nervous system is also situated in the ectoderm, and springs from the central nervous system, not by any observable nerve-trunks, but directly as a nervous plexus composed both of cells and fibres. Such a nei'vous plexus admits of being detected in the sub-umbrella of all Medusse, and in some species may be traced also into the tentacles. It invariably lies between the layer of muscle-fibre and that of the epithelium. The processes of neighbouring ganglion-cells in the plexus either coalesce or dwindle in their course to small fibres : at the marmn of the umbrella these 20 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. unite themselves with the elements of the nerve- lings. There are also described several peculiar tissue elements, such as, in the umbrella, nerve- fibiX3S which probably stand in connection with epithelium-cells ; nerve-cells which pass into muscle- fibres, similar to those which Kleinenberg has called neuro-muscular cells ; and, in the tentacles, neuro-muscular cells joined with cells of special sensation (Sinneszellen). No nervous elements could be detected in the convex surface of the umbrella, and it is doubtful whether they occur in the veil. In some species the nerve-fibres become aggre- gated in the region of the generative organs, and in that of the radial canals, thus giving rise in these localities to what may be called nerve-trunks. But in other species no such aggregations are apparent, the nervous plexus spreading out in the form of an even trellis-work. In the covered- eyed MedusiB the central nervous system consists of a series of separate centres which are not connected by any commissures. These nerve-centres are situated in the margin of the umbrella, and are generally eight in number, more rarely twelve, and in some species sixteen. They are thickenings of the ectoderm, which either enclose the bases of the sense-organs, or only cover the ventral side of the same. Histologically they consist of cells of special sensation, together with a thick layer of slender nerve-fibres. Ganglion- cells, however, are absent, so that the nerve-fibres are merely processes of epithelium- cells. STRUCTUKE OF THE MEDUS.E. 21 Drs. Hertwig made no observations on the peri- pheral nervous system of the covered-eyed Meckisse ; but they do not doubt that such a system would admit of being demonstrated, and intliis connection they cite the observations of Claus, who describes numerous ganglion-cells as occurring in the sub- umbrella of Chrysaora. Here I may appropriately state that before Drs. Hertwig had published their results, Professor Schiifer, F.R.S., conducted in my laborator}^ a careful research upon the histology of tlie Medusae, and succeeded in showing an intricate plexus of cells and fibres overspreading the sub-umbrella tissue of another covered-eyed Medusa (Aurelia aurita).* He also found that the marginal bodies present a peculiar modification of epithelium tissue, which is on its Avay, so to speak, towards becoming fully diff'erentiated into ganglionic cells. Lastly, returning to the researches of Drs. Hert- wig, these authors compare the nervous system of the naked-eyed with that of the covered-eyed Medusce, with the view of indicating the points which show the latter to be less developed than the former. These points are, that in the nerve- centres of the covered-eyed Medusae there are no true ganglion-cells, or only very few ; that the mass of the central nervous system is very small; and that the centralization of the nervous system is less complete in the one group than in the other. In their memoir these authors further supply much * See " Observations on the Nervous System of Aurelia aurita," Fhil. Trans., pt. ii., 1878. 3 22 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. interesting information toucliing tlie structure of the sense-organs in various species of Medusae ; but it seems scarcely necessary to extend the j)resent resume of their work by entering into this division of their subject. In a later publication, entitled " Der Organismus der Medusen und seine Stellung zur Keimbliitter- theorie," Drs. Hertwig treat of sundry features in the morphology of the Medusae which are of great theoretical importance ; but here again it would unduly extend the limits of the present treatise if I were to include all the ground which has been so ably cultivated by these industrious workei's. It will presently be seen in how striking a manner all the microscopical observations to which I have now briefly alluded are confirmed by the physiological observations — or, more correctly, I might say that the microscopical observations, in so far as they were concerned with demonstrating the existence of nerve-tissue in the Medusae, were fore- stalled by these physiological experiments ; for, with the exception of Professor Haeckel's work on Geryonidtie, they were all of later publication. But in matters of scientific inquiry mere priority is not of so much importance as it is too often supposed to be. Thus, in the present instance, no one of the workers was in any way assisted by the publications of another. In each case the work was independent and almost simultaneous. The remark just made applies also to the only research which still remains to be mentioned. This is the investigation undertaken and published by STRUCTURE OF THE MEDUSA. 28 Professor Eimer.* He began, like myself, by what in the next chapter I call the " fundamental obser- vation " on the effects of excising the nerve- centres, and from this basis he worked both at the physiology and the morphology of the neuro- muscular tissues. In point of time, I was the first to make the fundamental observation, and he was the first to publish it. The sundry features in which our subsequent investigations agreed, and those in which they differed, I shall mention throughout the course of the following pages. I shall now conclude this chapter by giving a brief account of those general principles of tlie physiology of nerve and muscle with which it is necessary to be fully acquainted, in order to under- stand the course of the following experiments. Nerve-tissue, then, universally consists of two elementary structures, viz. very minute nerve-cells and very minute nerve-fibres. The fibres proceed to and from the cells, so in some cases serving to unite the cells with one another, and in other cases with distant parts of the animal body. Nerve-cells are usually found collected together in aggregates, which are called nerve-centres or ganglia, to and from which large bundles cf nerve-fibres come and go. To explain the function of nerve- tissue, it is necessary to begin by explaining what physiologists mean by the term " excitability." Suppose that a * " Die Medusen physiologisch und morphologisch auf il.r NerveuBvstem untersucht " (Tixbingen, 1878). 24 JELLY-FISn, STAR-FISn, AND SEA.-URCHINS. muscle has been cut from the body of a freshly killed animal ; so long as it is not interfered with in any way, so long will it remain quite passive. But every time a stimulus is supplied to it, either by means of a pinch, a burn, an electrical shock, or a chemical irritant, the muscle will give a single contraction in response to every stimulation. And it is this readiness of organic tissues to respond to a suitable stimulus that physiologists designate by the term " excitability." Nerves, no less than muscles, present the pro- perty of being excitable. If, together with the excised muscle, there had been removed from the animal's body an attached nerve, every time any part of this nerve is stimulated the attached muscle will contract as before. But it must be carefully observed that there is this great difference between these two cases of response on the part of the muscle — that while in the former case the muscle responded to a stimulus applied directly to its own substance, in the latter case the muscle responded to a stimulus applied at a distance from its own substance, which stimulus was then conducted to the muscle by the nerve. And in this we perceive the characteristic function of nerve-^fer^s, viz. that of conducting stimuli to a distance. The function of nerve-ce^^s is different, viz. that of accumulating nervous energy, and, at fitting times, of discharging this energy into the attached nerve-fibres. The nervous energy, when thus discharged, acts as a stimulus to the nerve-fibre ; so that if a muscle is attached to the end of a fibre, it contracts on receiv- STRUCTURE OF THE MEDUSAE. 25 ing this stiiniilns. I may add that when nerve-cells are collected into ganglia, they often appear to discharge their energy spontaneously ; so that in all but the very lowest ^nimals, whenever we see apparently spontaneous action, we infer that ganglia are probably present. Lastly, another important distinction must be borne in mind — the distinction, namely, which is to be drawn between muscle and nerve. A stimulus applied to a nerveless muscle can only course through the muscle by giving rise to a visible wave of contraction, which spreads in all directions from the seat of disturbance as from a centre. A nerve, on the other hand, conducts the stimulus without sensibly moving or undergoing any change of shape. Now, in order not to forget this distinction, I shall always speak of muscle- fibres as conveying a visible wave of contraction, and of nerve- fibres as conveying an invisible, or molecular, wave of stinadation. Nerve-fibres, then, are functionally distinguished from muscle-fibres — and also from protoplasm — by displaying the pro- perty of conducting invisible, or molecular, waves of stimulation from one part of an organism to another, so establishing physiological continuity between such parts, without the necessary passage of waves of contraction. CHAPTER II. FUNDAMENTAL EXPERIMENTS. The naked-eyed Medusse are very much smaller in size than the covered-eyed, and as we shall find that the distribution of their nervous elements is somewhat different, it will be convenient to use different names for the large umbrella-shaped part of a covered-eyed Medusa, and the much smaller though corresponding part of a naked-eyed Medusa. The former, therefore, I shall call the umbrella, and the latter the swimming-bell, or nectocalyx. In each case alike this portion of the animal performs the office of locomotion, and it does so in the same way. I have already said that this mushroom-like organ, which constitutes the main bulk of the animal, is itself mainly constituted of thick trans- parent and non-contractile jelly, but that the whole of its concave surface is lined with a thin sheet of muscular tissue. Such being the structure of the organ, the mechanism w^hereby it effects locomotion is very simple, consisting merely of an alternate contraction and relaxation of the entire muscular sheet which lines the cavity of the bell. At each contraction of this muscular sheet the gelatinous FUNDAMENTAL EXPERIMENTS. 27 walls of the bell are drawn together ; the capacity of the bell being thus diminished, water is ejected from the open mouth of the bell backwards, and the consequent reaction propels the animal forwards. In these swimming movements, systole and diastole follow one another with as perfect a rhythm as they do in the beating of a heart. Effects of excising the entire Margins of Nectocalyces. Confining our attention under this heading to the naked-eyed Medusae, 1 find that the following proposition applies to every species of the group which I have as yet had the opportunity of ex- amining : Excision of the extreme margin of a nectocalyx causes immediate, total, and jyermanent 'paralysis of the entire organ. Nothing can possibly be more definite than in this highly remarkable efi'ect. I have made hundreds of observations upon various species of the naked-eyed Medusae, of all ages and conditions of freshness, vigour, etc. ; and I have constantly found that if the experiment be made with ordinary care, so as to avoid certain sources of error presently to be named, the result is as striking and decided as it is possible to desire.* Indeed, I do not know of any case in the animal kingdom where the removal of a centre of spon- taneity causes so sudden and so complete a paralysis * I have only met with one individual exception. This occurred in a specimen of Staurophora laciniata, where, after removal of the entire margin, three centres of spontaneity were found to i-emain in the sheet of contractile tissue lining the nectocalyx. 28 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. of the muscular system, there being no subsequent movements or twitchings of a reflex kind to disturb the absolute quiescence of the mutilated organism. The experiment is particularly beautiful if per- formed on Sarsia; for the members of this genus being remarkably active, the death-like stillness which results from the loss of so minute a portion of their substance is rendered by contrast the more surprising. From this experiment, therefore, I conclude that in the margin of all the species of naked-eyed Medusae which I have as yet had the opportunity of examining, there is situated an intensely localized system of centres of spontaneity, having at least for one of its functions the origination of impulses, to which the contractions of the nectocalyx, under ordinary circumstances, are exclusively due. And this obvious deduction is confirmed (if it can be conceived to require confirmation) by the behaviour of the sevej-ed margin. This continues its rhyth- mical contractions with a vigour and a pertinacity not in the least impaired by its severance from the main organism, so that the contrast between the perfectly motionless swimming-bell and the active contractions of the thread-like portion which has just been removed from its margin is as striking a contrast as it is possible to conceive. Hence it is not surprising that if the margin be left in situ, while other portions of the swimming-bell are mutilated to any extent, the spontaneity of the animal is not at all interfered with. For instance, if the equator of any individual belonging to the FUNDAMENTAL EXPERIMENTS. 29 genus Sarsia (Fig. 1) be cut completely through, so that the swimming-bell instead of being closed at the top is converted into an open tube, this open tube continues its rhythmical contractions for an indefinitely long time, notwithstanding that the organism so mutilated is, of course, unable to pro- gress. Thus it is a matter of no consequence how small or how large a portion of contractile tissue is left adhering to the severed margin of the swim- ming-bell ; for whether this portion be large or small, the locomotor centres contained in the margin are alike sufficient to supply the stimulus to contraction. Indeed, if only the tiniest piece of contractile tissue be left adhering to a single mar- ginal body cut out of the bell of Sarsia, this tiny piece of tissue, in this isolated state, will continue its contractions for hours, or even for days. Effects of excising the entire Margins of Umbrellas. Turning now to the covered-eyed division of the Medusie, I find, in all th^ species I have come across, that excision of the margins of umbrellas produces an effect analogous to that which is produced by excision of the margins of swimming-bells. There is an important difference, however, between the two cases, in that the paralyzing effect of the opera- tion on umbiellas is neither so certain nor so com- plete as it is on swimming-bells. That is to say, although in the majority of experiments such mutilation of umbrellas is followed by immediate paralysis, this is not invariably the case; so that so JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. one cannot here, as with the naked-eyed Medusae, predict with any great confidence what will be the immediate result of any particular experiment. Further, although such mutilation of an umbrella is usually followed by a paralysis as sudden and marked as that which follows such mutilation of a swimming-bell, the paralysis of the former differs from the paralysis of the latter, in that it is very seldom permanent. After periods varying from a few seconds to half an hour or more, occasional weak and unrhythmical contractions begin to mani- fest themselves, or the contractions may even be resumed with but little apparent change in their character and frequency. The condition of the animal before the operation, as to general vigour, etc., appears to be one factor in determining the effect of the operation ; but this is very far from being the only factor. Upon the whole, then, although in the species of covered-eyed Medusiie which I have as yet had the opportunity of examining, the effects which result from excising the margins of umbrellas are such as to warrant nie in saying that the main supply of locomotor centres appears to be usually situated in that part of these organs, these effects are neverthe- less such as to compel me at the same time to con- clude that the locomotor centres of the covered-eyed Medusc© are more diffused or seo'rea^ated than are those of the naked-eyed Medusse. Lastly, it should be stated that all the species of covered-eyed MedustB resemble all the species of naked-eyed Medusse, in that their members will endure any FUNDAMENTAL EXPERIMENTS. 31 amount of section it is possible to make upon any of their parts other than their margins without their spontaneity being in the smallest degree affected. Effects of excising Certain Portions of the Margins of Nectocalyces. The next question which naturally presents itself is as to whether the locomotor centres are equally distributed all round the margin of a swimming organ, or situated only, or chiefly, in the so-called marofinal bodies. To take the case of the naked- eyed Medusae first, it is evident that in most of the genera, in consequence of the intertentacular spaces being so small, it is impossible to cut out the marginal bodies (which are situated at the bases of the tentacles) without at the same time cutting out the intervening portions of the margin. The genus Sarsia, however, is admirably adapted (as a glance at Fig. 1 will show) for trying the effects of remov- ing the marginal bodies without injuring the rest of the margin, and vice versa. The results of such experiments upon members of this genus are as follow. Whatever be the condition of the individual operated upon as to freshness, vigour, etc., it endures excision of three of its marginal bodies without suffering any apparent detriment; but in most cases, as soon as the last marginal body is cut out, the animal falls to the bottom of the water quite motionless. If the subject of the experiment .32 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. happens to be a weakly specimen, it will, perhaps, never move again : it has been killed by something very much resembling nervous shock. On the other hand, if the specimen operated upon be one which is in a fresh and vigorous state, its period of quiescence will probably be but short ; the nervous shock, if we may so term it, although evidently considerable at the time, soon passes away, and the animal resumes its motions as before. In the great majority of cases, however, the activity of these motions is conspicuously diminished. The effect of excising all the marginal tissue from between the maro-inal bodies and leavinoj the latter untouched, is not so definite as is the effect of the converse experiment just described. Moreover, allowance must here be made for the fact that in this experiment the principal portion of the " veil" * is of necessity removed, so that it becomes impos- sible to decide how much of the enfeebling effect of the section is due to the removal of locomotor centres from the swimming-bell, and how much to a change in the merely mechanical conditions of the oro-an. From the fact, however, that excision of the entire margin of Sarsia produces total paralj^sis, while excision of the marginal bodies alone produces merely partial paralysis, there can be no doubt that both causes are combined. Indeed, it has been a matter of the greatest surprise to me how very minute a portion of the intertentacular marginal tissue is sufficient, in case of this genus, to animate the entire swimming-bell. Choosing * See Fiff. 1. FUNDAMENTAL EXPERIMENTS. 83 specimens of Sarsia, I have tried, by cutting out all the margin besides, to ascertain how minute a portion of intertentacular tissue is sufficient to perform this function, and I find that this portion may be so small as to be quite invisible without the aid of a lens. From numerous observations, then, upon Sarsia, I conclude that in this genus (and so, from analogy, \ robably in all the other genera of the true Medus[t3) locomotor centres are situated in every part of the extreme margin of a nectocalyx, but that there is a greater supply of such centres in the marginal bodies than elsewhere. Effects of excising Certain Portions of the Margin of Umhrellas. Coming now to the covered-eyed Medusae, I find that the concentration of the locomotor centres of the margin into the marginal bodies, or lithocysts, is still more decided than it is in the case of Sarsia. Taking Aurelia aurita as a type of the group, I cannot say that, either by excising the lithocysts alone or by leaving the lithocysts in situ and excising all the rest of the marginal tissue, T have ever detected the slightest indications of locomotor centres being present in any part of the margin of the umbrella other than the eight lithocysts ; so that all the remarks previously made upon this species, while we were dealing with the effects of excising the entire margin of umbrellas, are equally applicable to the experiment we are now consider- ing, viz. that of excising the lithocysts alone. In 34 JELLY-FISH, STJLB-FISH, JLN'D SEA-UBCHIXS. other words, but for the sake of symmetry, I might as well have stated at the first that in the case of the oovered-eyed Medusae all the remarkable para- Ivzintr effects which are obtaineeated, the response will be still more powerful, with a still shorter period of latency ; and so on, perhaps, for eight or ten stages, when the maximum force of contraction of which the tissue is capable will have been attained, while the period of latency wdll have been reduced to its minimum. This period is three-eighths of a second, or, in some cases, slightly less. Now, we have here a remarkable series of pheno- mena, and as it is a series which never fails to occur under the conditions named, I append tracings to give a better idea of the very marked and striking character of the results. The first tracing (Fig. 2) is a record of the successive increments of the responses to successive induction shocks of the same intensity, thrown in at three seconds' intervals — 54 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SKA-URCHINS. the cylinder being stationary during each response, and rotated a short distance with the hand during each interval of repose. The second tracing (Figs. 3 and 4) is a record of the difference between the lengths of the latent period, and also between the strengths of the con- traction, in the case {a) of the first of such a series of responses (Fig. 3), and (6) of the last of such a series (Fig. 4). From these tracings it will be manifest, without further comment, how surprising is the effect of a series of stimuli; first, in aroiisliKj the tissue, as it were, to increased activity, and, second, in developing a state of expectancy. In accordance with the now customary termi- nology, I shall call such a series of responses as are given in Fig. 2a" staircase." Such a staircase has a greater number of steps in it if caused by a weak current (compare Figs. 2 and 5) ; and if the strength of the current be suddenly increased after the maximum level of a staircase has been reached by using a feeble current, this level admits of being s'ightly raised (see Fig. 5). Moreover, I lind that a EXPERIMENTS IX SriMTLATIOX. o5 stimulus, ^vhicli at the bottom of a staircase is of less than minimal intensity, is al)le, at the top of a staircase, to give rise to a contraction of very nearly maximum intensity. That is to say, by employing 56 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FLSH, AND SEA-URCHINS. an induction stimulus of slightly less than minimal intensity in relation to the original irritability oi the tissue, no response is given to the first two or three shocks of a series ; but at the third or fourth shock a slight response is given, and from that point onward the staircase is built up as usual. This was the case in the experiment of which Fig. 2 is a record, no response having been given to the first two shocks. With regard to this interesting staircase action, two questions naturally present themselves. In the first place, we are anxious to know whether the arousing effect which is so conspicuous in a stair- case series is due to the occurrence of the previous stimulations, or to that of previous contractions; and, in the next place, we should like to know whether, during the natural rhythm of the tissue, each contraction exerts a beneficial influence on its successor, analogous to that which occurs in the case of contractions which are due to artijicial stimuli. To EXPERIMENTS IN STIMULATION. 57 answer the first of these questions, therefore, I built up a staircase in the ordinary way, and then sud- denly transferred the electrodes to the opposite side of the umbrella from that on which they rested while constructing the staircase. On now throwing in another shock at this part of the contractile tissue so remote from the part previously stimulated, the response was a maximum response. Similarly, if the electrodes were transferred in the way just described, not after the maximum effect had been attained, but at any point during the process of constructing a staircase, the response given to the next shock was of an intensity to make it rank as the next step in the staircase. Hence, shifting the position of the electrodes in no wise modifies the peculiar eff'ect we are considering ; and this fact conclusively proves that the effect is a general one, pervading the whole mass of the contractile tissue, and not confined to the locality whicli is the imme- diate seat of stimulation. Nevertheless, this fact does not tend to prove that the staircase effect depends on the process of contraction as distinguished from the process of stimulation, because the wave of the former process must always precede that of the latter. But, on the other hand, in this connection it is of the first importance to remember the fact already stated, viz. that a current which at the be- ginning of a series of stimulations is of slightly less than minimal intensity presently becomes minimal, and eventually of much more than minimal in- tensity— a staircase being thus built up of which the first observable step (or contraction) only occurs 58 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. in response to the second, tliird, or even fourth shock of the series. This fact conclusively proves that the staircase effect, at any rate at its com- mencement, depends on the process of stimulation as distinguished from that of contraction ; for it is obvious that the latter process cannot play any part in thus constructing what we may term the invisible steps of a staircase. To answer the second of the above questions, I placed an Aurelia with its concave suriace upper- most, and removed seven of its lithocysts ; I then observed the spontaneous discharge of the remain- ing one, and found it to be conspicuous enough that, after the occurrence of one of the natural pauses (if' this were of sufficient duration), the first contraction was feeble, the next stronger, the next still stronger, and so on, till the maximum was attained. This natural staircase action admits of being very prettily shown in another way. If a tolerably large Aurelia is cut into a spiral strip of small width and great length, and if all the lithocysts are removed except one at one end of the strip, it may be observed that, after the occurrence of a natural pause, the first discharge only penetrates perhaps about a quarter of the length of the strip, the next discharge pene- trates a little further, the next further, and so on, till finally the contraction waves pass from end to end. On now removing the ganglion, waiting a few minutes, and then stimulating with successive induction shocks, the same progressive penetration is observable as that which previously took place with the ganglionic stimulation. Lastly, the EXPERIMENTS IN STIMULATlOX. 59 identity of natural and artificial staircase action may be placed beyond all doubt by an experiment in which the effects of induction shocks and of ganglionic discharges are combined. To accomplish this, all the lithoc3^sts save one are removed, and a staircase is then built up in the ordinary way by successive induction shocks. It will now occasion- ally happen that the ganglion originates a discharge during the process of constructing the staircase, which is being built up by the artificial stimuli ; when this happens the resulting contraction takes its proper rank in the series, and this at whatever point the natural contraction happens to couie in. Thus, then, to summarize and conclude these observations, we have seen that if a single stimula- tion, whether of a natural or artificial kind, is supplied to the excitable tissues of a jelly-fish, a .short period, called the period of latency, will elapse, and then the jelly-fish will give a single weak contraction. If, as soon as the tissue has relaxed, the stimulation is again repeated, the period of latency will be somewhat shorter, and will be followed by a somewhat stronger contrac- tion. Similarly, if the stimulation is repeated a third time, the period of latency will be still shorter, and the ensuing contraction still stronger. And so on up to nine or ten times, w^hen the period of latency will be reduced to its rninimum, while the force of the contraction will be raised to its maximum; so that in the jelly-fish, the effect of a series of excitations supplied at short intervals from one another is that of both arousinof the tissue into GO JELLY-FJSH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-UHCUINS. a state of increased activity, and also of producing in it a state of greater expectancy. We have, more- over, seen that this effect depends upon the repe- ' tition of the process of stimulation, and not upon that of the process of contraction. Now, effects very similar to these have been found to occur in the case of the excitable plants by Dr. Burdon- Sanderson ; in the case of the frog's heart by Dr. Bowditch ; and in the case of reflex action of the spinal cord by Dr. Stirling. Indeed, the only difference in this respect between these four tissues, so widely separated from one another in the biological scale, consists in the time which may be allowed to elapse between the occurrence of the successive stimuli, in order to produce this so-called summating effect of one stimulus upon its successor: the memory, so to speak, of the heart-tissue for the occurrence of a former stimulus being longer than the memory of the jelly-fish tissue; while the memory of the latter is longer than that of the plant tissue. And I may here add that even in our own organization we may often observe the action of this principle of the summation of stimuli. For instance, we can . tolerate for a time the irritation caused by a crumb in the larynx, but very rapidly the sense of irrita- tion accumulates to a point at which it becomes impossible to avoid coughing. And similarly with tickling generally, the convulsive reflex movements to which it gives rise become more and more incon- t roll able the longer the stimulation is continued, until they reach a Tnaxim^um point, where, in EXPERIMENTS IN STIMULATION. CI persons susceptible to this kind of stimulation, the muscular action passes completely beyond the power of the will. Lastly, I may further observe, what I do not think has ever been observed before, that even in the domain of psychology the action of this principle admits of being clearly traced. We find it, for instance, in the rhythmical waves of emotion characteristic of grief, and at the other extreme we find it in the case of the ludicrous. We can endure for a short time, without giving any visible re- sponse, the psychological stimulation which is supplied by a comical spectacle; but if the latter continues sufficiently long in a sufficiently ludicrous manner, our appropriate emotion rapidly runs up to a point at which it becomes uncontrollable, and we burst into an explosion of ill-timed laughter. But in this case of psychological tickling, as in the previous case of physiological tickling, some persons are much more susceptible than others. ^Neverthe- less, there can be no doubt that from the excitable tissues of a plant, through those of a jelly-fish and a frog, up even to the most complex of our psj^cho- logical processes, we have in this recently discovered principle of the summation of stimuli a very remarkable uniformity of occurrence. Effects of Temjperatiire on Excitability. I shall now conclude this chapter with a brief statement of the effects of temperature on the excitability of the Medusae ; and before stating my results, I may observe that in all my experiments in this connection I changed the temperature of the 62 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISII, AND SEA-URCHINS. Medusae by drawing off the water in wliich tliey floated with a siphon, while at the same time I substituted w^ater of a different temperature from that which I thus abstracted. In this way, without modifying any of the other conditions to which the animals were exposed, I was able to observe the efiects of changing the temperature alone. With regard to the effect of temperature on the latent period of stimulation, the following table, setting forth the results of one among several experiments, explains itself Period of latent stimulation of the deganglio- nated tissues of Aurelia aurita as affected by tem- perature : — Temperature of water (Fahr.). 70- 35° 20° Period of latent stimulation. ^ second ^ second I second ^ second In the case of each observation, several shocks were administered before the latent period was taken, in order to decrease this period to its TiiinimuTn by the staircase action. When this is not done, the latent period at 20° may be as long as 1 J seconds ; but soon after this irritability disappears. The extraordinary sluggishness of the latent period at very low temperatures is fully equalled by the no less extraordinary sluggishness of the contraction. EXl'ElllMKNTS IN STIMULATION. 03 In order to render apparent the degree in which both these effects are produced, I here append a pair of tracings which were procured from the same piece of tissue when exposed to the different tem- peratures named. (N.B.— The seconds are wrongly i'iiX. 0. Fig. 7. marked in Fig. 7 ; they ought to he the same as in Fig. 6.) I may as well state here that in water at all temperatures, within the limits where responses to stimuli are given at all, the staircase action admits 64 JELLY-FISH, srAR-ilSH, AND SEA-UllCHINS. of being equally well produced ; but in order to procure the Tiiaxiinitm effect for any given tempera- ture, the rate at which the successive stimuli arc thrown in must be quicker in warm than in cold water. CHAPTER IV. KXPERIMENTS IN SECTION OF COVEllED-EYED MEDUSA Amount of Section which the Neiiro-muscular Tissues of the Medusoe will endure without suffering Loss of their Physiological Continuity. The extent to which the neuro-muscular tissues of the Medusa may be mutilated without under- going destruction of their physiological continuity is in the highest degree astonishing. For instance, to begin with the covered-eyed Medusae, I shall briefly state three modes of section, the results of which serve to show in a striking manner the fact in question. The annexed woodcuts represent the umbrella of Aurelia aurita, with its manubrium cut off at the base, and the under or concave surface of the umbrella exposed to view, shewing in the centre the ovaries, and radiating from them the branched system of nutrient tubes. The umbrella when fully expanded, as here represented, is about the size of a soup plate, and, as previously stated, all the marginal ganglia are aggregated in the eight marginal bodies or lithocysts. Therefore if the reader will imagine the first of the diagrams (Fig. 8) no JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. to be overspread with a disc of muslin, the fibres and mesh of which are finer than those of the finest and closest cobweb, and if he will imagine the mesh of these fil)res to s^art from these maroinal oranglia, he will gain a tolerably correct idea of the lowest nervous system in the animal kingdom. Now, suppose that seven of these eight ganglia are cut out, the remaining one then continues to supply its rhythmical discharges to the muscular sheet of the SECTION OF COVERED-EYED MEDUSA. G7 bell, the result being, at each discharge, two con- traction waves, which start at the same instant, one on each side of the ganglion, and which then course with equal rapidity in op])osite directions, mm' m Fig. 9. and so meet at the point of the disc which is opposite to the ganglion. Suppose, now, a number of radial cuts are made in the disc according to such a plan as this (Fig. 9), wherein every radial cut deeply overlaps those on either side of it. The 68 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. contraction waves which now originate from the ganglion must either become blocked and cease to pass round the disc, or they must zigzag round and round the tops of these overlapping cuts. Now, remembering that the passage of these contraction waves is presumably dependent on the nervous network progressively distributing the ganglionic impulse to the muscular fibres, surely we should expect that two or three overlapping cuts, by completely severing all the nerve-fibres lying between them, ought to destroy the functional continuity of these fibres, and so to block the passage of the contraction wave. Yet this is not the case ; for even in a specimen of Aurelia so severely cut as the one here represented, the contraction waves, starting from the ganglion, continued to zigfzao- round and round the entire series of sections. The second mode of section to which I have alluded is as follows (Fig. 10). The central circle {x) stands for an open space cut out of the umbrella ; the outer circle indicates the margin of the animal, with all lithocysts save one (I) removed ; and the median circular line represents a cut. It will be seen that the effect of this cut is almost completely to sever the mass of tissue at z from the rest of the umbrella, the only connection between them being the narrow neck of tissue at z. Yet, in the case to which I refer, the contraction waves emanating from I passed in the directions represented by the arrows without undergoing any appreciable loss of vigour. Upon completing the circular cut at z, the ring of tissue (y z) became totally paralyzed, while SECTION OF COVERED-EYED MEDUSA. 69 the outer circle, of course, continued its contractions as before. Now, the neck of tissue at z measured only one-eighth of an inch across, while the ring of tissue {y z), when cut through and straightened °out Fig. 10. upon the table, measured one inch across and sixteen inches in length ; that is to say, sixteen square inches of tissue derived its impulse to vigorous contrac- tions through a channel one-eighth of an inch wide. 70 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, Ax\D SEA- URCHINS. notwithstanding that the latter was situated at the furthest point of the circle from the discharging ^miMwmmw ^m^^^ Fig. 11. lithocyst which the form of the section rendered possible. SECTION OF COVERED-EYED MEDUSA 71 Lastly, the third mode of section is represented in the next cut. Here seven of the marginal ganglia having been removed as before, the eighth one was made the point of origin of a circumferential section, which was then carried round and round the bell in the form of a continuous spiral — the result, of course, being this long ribbon-shaped strip of tissue with the ganglion at one end and the remainder of the swimming-bell at the other. Well, as before, the contraction-waves always originated at the ganglion ; but now they had to course all the way along the strip until they arrived at its other ex- tremity; and, as each wave arrived at that extremity, it delivered its influence into the remainder of the swimming-bell, which thereupon contracted. Now, in this experiment, when the spiral strip is only made about half an inch broad, it may be made more than a yard long before all the bell is used up in making the strip; and as nothing can well be imagined as more destructive of the continuity of a nerve-plexus than this spiral mode of section must be, we cannot but regard it as a very remark- able fact that the nerve-plexus should still continue to discharge its function. Indeed, so remarkable does this fact appear, that to avoid accepting it we may well feel inclined to resort to another hypothesis, namely, that these contraction- waves do not depend for their passage on the nervous network at all, but that they are of the nature of the muscle waves, or of the waves which we see in undifferentiated protoplasm, where all parts of the mass being equally excitable and equally contractile, however severely 72 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. we cut the mass, as long as we do not actually divide it, contraction-waves will pass throughout the whole mass. But this very reasonable hypo- thesis of the contraction-waves in the Medusae being possibly nothing more than muscle- waves is negatived by other facts, which I shall now proceed to state. In the first place, if a number of experiments be tried in any of the three modes of section above described, it will be found that extreme variations are manifested as regards the degree of tolerance. In the spiral mode of section, for instance, it will sometimes happen that the contraction-wave will become blocked when the contractile strip is only an inch lono- while in other cases (as in the one represented) the wave will continue to pass through a strip more than thirty times that length ; and between these two extremes there are all possible grades of tolerance. Now it seems to me that if the tissue through which these contraction-waves pass is supposed (as far as they are concerned) to be of a functionally homogeneous nature, no reason can be assigned why there should be such great differences in the endurance of the tissue in different individual cases ; while, if we suppose that the passage of the contraction-waves is more or less dependent on the functional activity of the nervous plexus which we know from microscopical examina- tion to be present, Ave encounter no such difficulty ; for it is almost to be expected that in some cases it would happen that important nerves would soon be encountered by the section, while in other cases it SECTION OF COVERED-EYED MEDUSiE. 73 would happen that such nerves would escape the section for a longer distance. It is indeed incredible that anyone nerve should happen to pursue a spiral course twice or thrice round the umbrella, and at the same time happen to be concentric witli the course pursued by the section ; but, as we shall presently see, such an hy})othcsis as this is not necessary to account for the facts. Again, in the second place, strong evidence that the passage of the contraction-waves is dependent on the functional activity of the nervous plexus, and therefore that they are not merely muscle- waves, is furnished by the fact that at whatever point in a spiral strip which is being progressively elongated by section the contraction-wave becomes blocked, the blocking is sure to take place coinjMely and exclusively at that point. Now, as I have tried this experiment a great number of times, and always tried it by carefully feeling the way round {i.e. only making a very short continuation of the cut after the occurrence of each con tract ion- wave, and so ver}^ precisely localizing the spot at which the contraction- wave ceased to pass), I can scarcely doubt that in every case the blocking is caused by the cutting through of nerves.* * In a liighly interesting paper recently published by Dr. W. H. Gaskell, F.K.S., on " The Innervation of the Heart" (Journ. tf Physiol. y vol. iv. p. 43, et seq.), it is shown that the experiments in section thus far described yield strikingly similar results when performed upon the heart of the tortoise and the heart of the skate. Dr. Gaskell inclines to the belief that in these cases the contraction-waves are merely muscle-waves. There is one important fact, however, which even here seems to me to indicate 74} JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISII, AND SEA-URCHINS. But, lastly, the strongest evidence in favour of this view as affoi-Jed by the following observations. At the beginning of this treatise I stated that the distinguishing function of nerve consists in its power of conducting stimuli to a distance, irrespective of the passage of a contraction- wave ; and I may here add that when a stimulus so conducted reaches a ganglion, or nerve-centre, it causes the ganglion to discharge by so-called " reflex action." Now, this distinguishing function of nerve can plainly be proved to be present in the Medusae. For instance, take such a section of Aurelia as this one (Fig. 12), that the propai^ation of the wave is at least in some measure dependent on nervons conduction. This fact is, that after a contraction-wave has been blocked by the severity of a spiral or other form of section, it may again be made to force a passage under the influence of vagus stimulatiou. Moreover, in a paper still more recently published by Drs. Brunton and Cash on " Electrical Stimulation of the Fi'og's Heart" (Proc. Roy. 8')C., vol. XXXV., No. 227, p. 455, et seq.) it is remarked, " Another interesting consideration is, whether the stimulus which each cavity of the heart transmits to the succeeding one consists in the propagation of an actual muscular wave, or in the propagation of an impulse along the nerves. The observations of Gaskell have given very great importance to the muscular wave occurring in each cavity of the heart of cold-blooded animals as a stimulus to the contraction of the next succeeding cavity. Our observations appear to us to show that, while this is an important factor, it is not the only one in the tiansmission of stimuli. . . . We consider that stimuli are ahso propagated from one chamber of the heart to another through nervous channels : thus we find that irritation of the venus sinus will sometimes produce simultaneous contractions of the auricle and ventricle, ixastead of the ventricular beat succeeding the auricular in the ordinary way. This we think is hardly consistent with the hypo- thesis, that a stimulus consists of the propagation of a muscular wave only from the auricle to the ventricle." SECTION OF COVERED-EYED MEDUSJE. /o wherein the bell has been cut into the form of a continuous parallelogram of tissue with the ovaries and a single remaining ganglion at one end. (The cuts interposed in the parallelogram may, for the present, be disregarded.) Novv% if the end mai ked a 76 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHIXS. of the neuro-muscular sheet most remote from the ganglion be gently brushed with a camel's hair brush — i.e. too gently to start a responsive contrac- tion-wave— the ganglion at the other end will shortly afterwards discharge, as shown by its start- incr a contraction-wave at its own end of the parallelogram, h; thus proving that the stimulus caused by brushing the tissue at the other end, a, must have been conducted all the way along the parallelogram to the terminal ganglion, 6, so causing the terminal crano^lion to discharcje bv reflex action. Indeed, in many cases, the passage of this nervous wave of stimulation admits of being seen. For the numberless tentxicles which frin^re the maro-in of Aurelia are more higlily excitable than is the general contractile tissue of the bell; so that on brushing the end a of the parallelogram remote from the ganglion, the tentacles at this end respond to the stimulus by a contraction, then those next in the series do the same, and so on — a wave of con- traction being thus set up in the tentacular fringe, the passage of which is determined by the passage of the nervous wave of stimulation in the super- jacent nervous netwoik. This tentacular wave is in the illustration represented as having travelled nearlv half the whole distance to the terminal o-ancr- lion, and when it reaches that ganglion it will cause it to discharge by reflex action, so giving rise to a visible wave of muscular contraction passing in the direction h a, opposite to that which the nervous or tentacular wave had previously pursued. Now this tentacular wave, being an optical expression of a pas- SECTION OF COVER ED- EYED MEDUSAE. 77 sage of a wave of stimulation, is a siglit as beautiful as it is unique ; and it affords a first-rate oppor- tunity of settling tlds all-important (question, namely, Will this conductile or nervous function prove itself as tolerant towards a section of the tissue as the contractile or muscular function has already proved itself to be ? For, if so, we shall gain nothing on the side of simplicity by assuming that the contract ^on-^yilveii are merely muscle- waves, so long as the conduction or unclouhfcclly nervous waves are equal 1}^ able to pass round sections interposed in their path. Briefly, then, I find that the nervous waves of stimulation are quite as able to pass round these interposed sections as are the waves of con- traction. Thus, for instance, in this specimen (Fig. 12), the tentacular wave of stimulation continued to pass as before, even after I had submitted the parallelogram of tissue to the tremendously severe form of section which is represented in the illustra- tion; and this fact, in my opinion, is one of the most important that has been brought to light in the whole range of invertebrate physiology. For what does it prove ? It proves that the distinguishing function of nerve, where it first appears upon the scene of life, admits of being performed vicariously to almost any extent by all parts of the same tissue-mass. If we revert to our old illustration of the muslin as representing the nerve-plexus, it is clear that, how- ever much we choose to cut the sheet of muslin with such radial or spiral sections as are represented in the illustrations, one could alwa^'s trace the threads of the muslin with a needle round and round the 78 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. disc, without once interrupting the continuity of the tracing; for on coming to the end of a divided thread, one could always double back on it and choose another thread which might be running in the required direction. And this is what we are now compelled to believe takes place in the fibres of this nervous network, if we assume that these visible fibres are the only condactile elements which are present. Whenever a stimulus wave readies a cut, we must conclude that it doubles back and passes into the neighbouring fibres, and so on, time after time, till it succeeds in passing round and round any number of overlapping cuts. This is, no doubt, as I have already observed, a very remarkable fact ; but it becomes still more so when we have regard to the histological researches of Professor Schafer on the structural character of this nerve-plexus. For these researches have shown that the nerve-fibres which so thickly overspread the muscular sheet of Aurelia do not constitute a true plexus, but that each fibre is comparatively short and nowhere joins with any of the other fibres; that is to say, although the constituent fibres of the network cross and recross one another in all directions — sometimes, indeed, twisting round one another like the strands of a rope — they can never be actually seen to join, but remain anatomi- cally insulated throughout their length. So that the simile by which I have represented this nervous network — the simile, namely, of a sheet of muslin overspreading the whole of the muscular sheet — is, as a simile, even more accurate than has hitherto SECTION OF COVERED-EYED MEDUSA. 79 appeared ; for just as in a piece of muslin the con- stituent threads, although frequently meeting one another, never actually coalesce, so in the nervous network of Aurelia, the constituent fibres, although frequently in contact, never actually unite. Now, if it is a remarkable fact that in a fully differentiated nervous network the constituent fibres are not improbably capable of vicarious action to almost any extent, much more remarkable does this fact become when we find that no two of these constituent nerve-fibi-es are histologically continuous with one another. Indeed, it seems to me we have here a fact as startling as it is novel. There can scarcely be any doubt that some influence is com- municated from a stimulated fibre a to the adjacent fibre h at the point where these fibres come into close apposition. But what the nature of the process may be whereb}^ a disturbance in the excitable protoplasm of a sets up a sympathetic disturbance in the anatomically separate protoplasm of 6, supposing it to be really such — this is a question concerning which it would as yet be premature to speculate. But I think it may be well for physiologists to keep awake to the fact that a process of this kind probably takes place in the case of these nerve-fibres. For it thus becomes a possibility which ought not to be overlooked, that in the fibres of the spinal cord, and in ganglia generally, where hi.stologists have hitherto been un- able to trace any anatomical or structural continuity between cells and fibres, which must nevertheless be supposed to possess physiological or functional 80 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. continuity — it thus becomes a possibility that in these cases no such anatomical continuity exists, but that the pliysiological continuity is maintained by some such process of physiological induction as probably takes place among the nerve-fibres of Aurelia.* I have now to detail another fact of a very puzzling nature, but one which is certainly of importance. When the spiral section is performed on Aurelia auvita, and when, as a consequence, the contraction-waves which traverse the elongating strip become at some point suddenly blocked, if the section be stopped at this point it not unfrequently happens that after a time the blocking suddenly ceases, the contraction -waves again passing from the strip into the umbrella as freely as they did before the section reached the point at which the blocking occurred. The time required for this restoration of physiological continuity is very variable, the limits being from a few seconds to an hour or more ; usually, however, it is from two to four minutes. This process of i e-establishing the physiological connections, although rapid, is not so instantaneous as is that of their destruction by section. In general it requires the passage of several contraction- waves before the barrier to the passage of succeeding waves is completely thrown * That it can scarcely be electrical induction would seem to be shown by the fact that such effects can only be produced on nerves by strong currents, and also by the fact that the saline tissuesof the swimming.bell must sTiort-circuit any feeble electncal currents as soon as they are genoratnd. SECTION OF COVERED-EYED MEDUS.E. 81 down. The first wave wliich effects a passage appears to have nearly all its force expended in overcoming the barrier, the residue being only sufficient to cause a very feeble, and sometimes almost imperceptible, contraction of the umbrella. The next Avave, however, passes across the barrier with more facility, so that the resulting contraction of the umbrella is more decided. The third wave, again, causes a still more pronounced contraction of the umbrella ; and so on with all succeeding waves, until every trace of the previous blocking has disappeared. When this is the case, it generally happens that the strip will again admit of being elongated for a short distance before a blocking of the contract! on- waves again supervenes. Sometimes it will be found that this second blockage will also be overcome, and that the strip will then admit of being still further elongated without the passage of the waves being obstructed ; and so on occa- sionally for three or four stages. The same series of phenomena may be shown in another way. If a contractile strip of tolerable length be obtained, with the waves passing freely from one end to the other, and if a series of parallel and equidistant cuts be made along one side of the strip, in a direction at right angles to the length, and each cut extending two-thirds of the breadth of the strip, the chances are in favour of the con- traction-waves being wholly unaffected by the sec- tions, however numerous these may be. But now, if another series of parallel and equidistant cuts of the same length as the first ones, and alternating with 82 JELLY-FISn, STAR-FlSir, AND SEA-URCHINS. them, be made along the other side of tlie contractile strip, the result is, of course, a number of interdigi- tating cuts ; and it is easy to see that by beginning with a few such cuts and progressively increasing their number, a point must somewhere be reached at which one portion will become physiologically separated from the rest. The amount of such section, however, Avhich contractile strips will some- times endure is truly surprising. I have seen such a strip twenty inches long by one and a half inches wide with ten such cuts along each side, and the contraction- waves passing without impediment from end to end. But what I wish more especially to observe just now is, that by progressively increasing the number of such interdigitating cuts up to the point at which the contraction- wave is blocked, and then leaving the tissue to recover itself, in many cases it will be observed that the blocking is sooner or later overcome; that on then adding more interdigitating cuts the blocking again supervenes ; but that in time it may again be overcome, and so on. It is, however, comparatively rare to find cases in which blocking is overcome twice or thrice in succession. Section is not the only way in which blocking of waves may be caused in contractile strips. I find that pressure, even though very gentle, exerted on any part of a strip causes a blocking of the waves at that part, even after the pressure has been removed. If the pressure has been long continued, after its removal the blocking will probably be permanent; but if the pressure has been only of SECTION OF COVER KD-EYED MEDUSAE. 83 short duration, the blocking will most likely be transitory. Even the slight btrains caused by hand- ling a contractile strip in the air are generally followed by a decrease in the rate of tlie waves, and sometimes by their being completely blocked. Other methods by which the passage of weaves in contrac- tile strips admits of being blocked will be alluded to farther on. Now, in all these cases of temporary blocking we must conclude that when the contraction-waves succeed in at last forcing a passage, some structural change has taken place in the tissue at the region of injury, corresponding with the functional change of the re-establishment of physiological continuity. The waves previously stopped at a certain point of section or otherwise, after beating for a time on the physiological barrier, are at last able to throw doAvn the barrier, and thenceforward to proceed on their way unhindered. What, then, is the nature of the structural change which has taken place ? In the early days of this research, before the presence of a nerve-plexus had been proved histo- logically, I argued in favour of such a plexus on the grounds furnished by many of the foregoing ex- periments; and at a lecture given in the Royal Institution I ventured to say that if a careful investigation of the histology of these tissues should fail to show the plexus which the result of those experiments required me to assume, we should still be compelled to suppose that the plexus w^as pre- sent, although not sufficiently differentiated lo admit of being seen. I further ventured to suggest 84 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. that in this event the facts just stated might be taken to substantiate the theory of Mr. Herbert Spencer* on the genesis of nerve- tissue in general. This theory is that which supposes incipient conductile tissues, or rudimentary nerve-fibres, to be differ- entiated from the surrounding contractile tissues, or homogeneous protoplasm, by a process of integra- tion which is due simply to use; so that just as water continually widens and deepens the channel through which it flows, so molecular or nervous waves of stimulation, by always flowing through the same tissue-tract^, tend ever more and more to excavate for themselves functionally differentiated lines of passage. Such being Mr. Spencer's theory, I applied it hypothetically to the above facts in the words which I may here quote. "As the successive waves beat rhythmically on the area of obstruction, more or less of the mole- cular disturbances must every time be equalized throuQfh these lines of discharo-e, which from the first have been almost sufficient to maintain the physiological continuity of the tissue. Therefore, according to the hypothesis, every wave that is blocked imposes upon these particular lines of dis- charge a much higher degree of functional activity than they were ever before required to exercise ; and this greater activity causing in its turn greater permeability, a point will sooner or later arrive at v/hich these lines of discharge, from having been almost, become quite able to draft off sufficient molecular motion, or stimulating influence, to carry SECTION OF COVERED-EYED MEDUSA. 85 on the contraction-waves beyond the areas of previous blocking. In such instances, of course, v^^e should expect to find wliat I always observed to be the case, viz. that the first contraction- wave which passes the barrier is only very feeble, the next stronger, the next still stronger, and so on, accord- ing as the new passage becomes more and more per- meable by use, until at last the contract ion- waves pour over the original barrier without any per- ceptible diminution of their force. In some cases, by exploring with graduated stimuli and needle- point terminals, I was able to ascertain the precise line through which this eruption of stimulating influence had taken place." I have now to state the effect upon this hypo- thesis which in my opinion has been produced by the histological proof that the plexus in question is composed of fully differentiated nerves. Briefly, then, I think that the hypothesis still holds to the extent of being the only one available whereby to explain the facts ; but there is this great difference, viz. that the hypothesis need not now be applied to the genesis of nerve-tissue out of comparatively undifferentiated contractile tissue, but rather to the increasing of the functional activity of already well- differentiated nerve-tissue. In other words, we have not now to suppose that nerve-tissue is formed de novo in the region of blocking ; but, in my opinion, we still have to suppose that the nerve- libres which were already there have their func- tional capabilities so far improved by the greater demand imposed upon them, that whereas at first 86 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. they were not able, eventually they became able to draft off enough molecular disturbance to carry on a stimulus adequate to cause a muscular contraction. It will be observed that the difference thus ex- pressed is one of considerable importance; for now the facts cease to lend any countenance to Mr. Spencer's theory touching the formation of nerves out of protoplasm, or other contractile material. They continue, however, to countenance his views touching the improvement of functional capacity which nerve-fibres, when already formed, undergo by use; and this, wliich is in itself an important matter, is the point with which I was mainly con- cerned in the lecture of the Royal Institution just alluded to. For, as I then observed, in this theory of nerve-fibres becoming more and more functionally developed by use, we probably have a physical ex- planation, which is as full and complete as such an explanation can ever be, of the genesis of mind. " For from the time that intelligence first dawned upon the scene of life, whenever a new relation had to be established in the region of mind, it could only be so established in virtue of some new line of dis- charge being excavated through the substance of the brain. The more often this relation had to be repeated in the mind, the more often would this discharge require to take place in the brain, and so the more easy would every repetition of the process become. . . . Thus it is, according to the theory, that there is always a precise proportion between the constancy with which any relations have been joined together during the history of SECTION OF COVEKED-EYED MEDUS.E. 87 intelligence, and the difficulty which intelligence now experiences in trying to conceive of such relations as disjoined. Thus it is that, even during the history of an individual intelligence, 'practice makes perfect,' by frequently repeating the needful stimulations along the same lines of cerebral dis- charge, so rendering the latter ever more and more permeable by use. Thus it is that a child learns its lessons by frequently repeating them ; and thus it is that all our knowledge is accumulated."* Rate of Transmission of Stiniull. The rate at which contraction-waves traverse spiral strips of Aurelia is variable. It is largely determined by the length and width of the strip ; so * I have associated the above theory of nerve-genesis with the name of Mr. Spencer, because it occupies so prominent a place in his " Principles of Psychology." But from what I have said in the text, I think it is clear that the theory, as presented by Mr. Spencer, consists of two essentially distinct hypotheses — the one relating to the formation of nerve-tissue out of protoplasm, and the other to the increase of functional capacity in a nerve-fibre by use (a third hypothesis of Mr. Spencer relating to the formation of ganglion-tissue does not here concern us). The latter hypothesis, however, ought not to be associated with Mr. Spencer's name without explaining that it has likewise occurred to other writers, the first of which, so far as I can ascertain, was La- marck, who says, "Dans toute action, le fluide des nerfs qui la provoque, subit un mouvement de deplacement qui y donne lieu. Or, lorsque cette action a ete plusieurs fois repe'tce, il n'est pas douteux que le fluid qui I'a exe'cutee, ne se soit fraye une route, qui lui devient alors d'autanfc plus facile a parcourir, qu'il l*a effectivement plus souvant francliie, et qu'il n'ait lui-meme une aptitude plus grand a suivre cette route frayee que celles qui le sunt niuins." ("Phil. ZooL," torn ii. pp. 318-19.) 88 JKLLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-UPtCHlNS. that the best form of strip to use for the purpose of ascertaining the inaximum rate is one which I shall call the circular strip. A circular strip is obtained by first cutting out the central bodies {i.e. manu- brium and ovaries), and then, with a single radial cut, converting the animal from the form of an open ring to that of a continuous band. I dis- tinguish this by the name " circular " band or strip, because the two ends tend to preserve their original relative positions, so giving the strip more or less of a circular form. Such a strip has the advantage of presenting all the contractile tissue of the swimming-bell in one continuous band of the greatest possible width, and is therefore the form of strip that yields the maximum rate at which contraction-waves are able to pass. The reason why the "inaxirtiiim rate should be the one sought for is because this is the rate which must most nearly approximate the natural rate of contraction- waves in the unmutilated animal. This rate, at the temperature of the seaand with vigorous specimens, I find to be eighteen inches per second. In a circular strip the rate of the waves is uniform over the whole extent of the strip ; so that the time of their transit from one point to another varies directly as the length of the strip. But on now narrowing such a strip, although the rate is thus slowed, the relation between the narrowing and the slowing is not nearly so precise as to admit of our saying that the rate varies inversely as the width. The following figure will serve to show the propor- tional extent to which the passage of contraction- StX'TION OF COVKUIiD-KVKI) MKDrS.E. 89 waves is retarded by narrowing the area through which they pass : — Time from oiul to end of a circular stiip Time after width has been reduced to one-half Time after width has been reduced to one-quarter... Time after width has been reduced to one-eighth ... In such experiments it generally happens, as here represented, that reducing the width of a circular strip by one-half produces no effect, or only a sli. ht effect, on the rate, while further narrowing to the degree mentioned produces a conspicuous effect. I may also state that if, as occasionally happens, the immediate effect of narrowing a circular strip to one- half is to temporarily block the contraction-waves, when the latter again force their passage, their rate is sloAver than it was before. It seems as if the more pervious tissue tracts having been destroyed by the section, the less pervious ones, though still able to convey the contraction-wave, are not able to convey it so rapidly as were the more pervious tracts. In order to ascertain whether certain zones of the circular contractile sheet in all individuals habitually convey more of the contractile influence than do other zones, I tried a number of experiments in the following form of section. Having made a circular strip, I removed all the lithocysts save one, and then cut the strip as represented in Fig. 14. On 90 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. now stimulating the end a, or on watching the lithocyst there discharge, the resulting contraction- wave would be observed to bifurcate at h, and then pass on as two separate waves through the zones, h c,b d. Now, as these two waves were started at SECTION OF COVERKD-EYED MEDUS.^. 91 the same instant of time, tliey ran, as it were, a race in the two zones, and in this way the eye could judge with perfect ease which wave occupied the shortest time in reaching its destination. This ex- periment couUl be varied by again bisecting each of these two zones, thus making four zones in all, and four waves to run in each race. A number of experiments of this kind showed me that there is no constancy in the relative conductivity of the same zones in different individuals. In some in- stances, the waves occupy less time in passing through the zone h c than in passing through the zone b d ; in other instances, the time in the two zones is equal ; and, lastly, the converse of the first-mentioned case is of equally frequent occur- rence. Very often the waves become blocked in h c, while ihej continue to pass in h d, and vice versa. Now, all these various cases are what we might expect to occur, in view of the variable points at which contraction- waves become blocked in spiral strips, etc. ; for if the contractile tissues are not functionally homogeneous, and if the rela- tively pervious conductile tracts are not constant as to their position in different individuals, the results I have just described are the only ones that could be yielded by the experiments in question. Considering, however, that in these experiments the central zones are not so long as the peripheral zones, I think it may fairly be said that the con- ductile power of the latter is greater than that of the former; for, otherwise, the above experiments ought to yield a large majority of races won by the 92 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. waves that course through the central zones, and this is not the case. Indeed, it is surprising how often the race is, as it were, neck and neck, thus showing that the relative conductivity of all the zones is precisely adjusted to their relative lengths ; and forasmuch as in the unmutilated animal this adjustment must clearly serve the purpose of securing to the con tractii)n- wave a passage of uniform rate over the whole radius of the umbrella, I doubt not that, if it were possible to perform the race-course section without interrupting any of the lines of conduction-tissue, neck and neck races would be of invariable occurrence. Interdigitating cuts, as might be expected, pro- long the time of contraction-waves in their passage through the tissue in which the cuts are interposed. For example, in a spiral strip measuring twenty-six inches in length, the time required for the passage of a contraction -wave from one end to the other is represented by the line a h in the annexed woodcut. But after twenty interdigitating cuts had been interposed, ten on each side of the strip, the time increased to c d, the line e f Fig. 15. representing one second. And more severe forms of section are, of course, attended with a still more retarding influence. The effects of temperature on the rate of con- traction-waves are very striking. For instance, in a rather narrow strip measuring twenty- eight inches hmg and one and a half inches wide, the following variations in rate occurred : — SECTION OF COVERED-EYED MEDUS.E. 03 Temperature of water. Time occnpiod in pass ge of contractile waves. 26° 32° 42° 65° 75° 85° 4 seconds. 3 seconds. 2f seconds. 2 seconds. 1|- seconds. Blocked. Or,ad()pting again thegraphic method ofstateiuent, these vai'iations may be represented as follows : — 26° 32° 42 65° 75° 85° One second ... ^^_^^^^^_^^^ Fig. 16. Submitting a contractile strip to slight strains has also the effect of retarding the rate of the waves while they pass through the portions of the strip which have been submitted to strain. The method of straining which I adopted was to pass my finger below the strip, and then, by raising my hand, to bring a portion of the strip slightly above the level of the water. The in i table or contractile surface was kept uppermost, and therefore suffered a gentle strain ; for the weight of the tissue on either side of the finger made the upper surface somewhat convex. By passing the finger all the way along 94< JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. the strip in this way, the latter might be gently strained througliout its entire length, the degree of straining being determined by the height out of the water to which the tissue was raised. Of course, if the strip is too greatly strained, the contraction- waves become blocked altogether, as described above ; but shortly before this degree of straining was reached, I could generally observe that the rate of the waves was diminished. To give one instance, a contractile strip measuring twenty -two inches had the rate of its waves taken before and after strain- ing of the kind described. The result was as follows : — Befoye straining... After straining .., One second Fig. 17. Immediately after severe handling of this kind, the retardation of contraction-waves is sometimes even more marked than here represented; but I think this may be paitly due to shock, for on srivino; the tissue a little while to recover, the rate of the waves becomes slightly increased. Ansesthetics likewise have the effect of slowing the rate of contraction-waves before blocking them. Taking, for instance, the case of chloroform, a narrow spiral strip between one and two feet long was immersed in sea-water containing a large dose of the anaesthetic ; the observations being taken at six seconds' intervals, the following were the results: — SECTION OF COVKRKD-EYKl) MEDUSAE. Normal wiitor Six secmiils after trans- ference to chloroform Six seconds later Six seconds later S i X seconds later Six seconds later One second In such experiniL^nts, the recovery of the normal rate in unpoisoned water is gradual. Taking, for instance, the case of a spiral strip in morphia (Fig. 19), it will be seen that the original rate did not fully return. Some substances, however, exert a more marked permanent effect of this kind than do weak solutions of morphia. Here, for instance, is an experiment with alcohol (see Fig. 20). In normal water yuart?r of an hour after exposure to mopliia ... One iniiiuieaft'.T strength- ening dose Four uiiiiut'-s later, and just bel'on- blocking of wave Fifteen seconds lat r,wave cimtinuing blocked Innned lately alter pa-s.ige of wave on r- toation to nui inal sea-water . . . Four minutes later Quarter of an hour later... An hour later One second ... .,. ^^^--—^^—^■■■i™ Fig. la. 96 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS In normal water Quarter of an hour after o.\posure to weak dose ... Two minutes after strength- ening of dose Five minutes later, and just before blocking of wave Fifteen seconds later, wave continuing blocked Immediately after passage of wave on restoration to normal sea-water An hour later One second Fig. 20. From these experiments, however, it must not be definitely concluded that it is the antesthesiating property of such substances which exerts this slow- ing and blocking influence on con traction- waves, for 1 find that almost any foreign substance, whether or not an anaesthetic, will do the same. That nitrite of amyl, cafiein, etc., should do so, one would not be very surprised to hear ; but it might not so readily be expected that strychnine, for instance, should block contraction- waves ; yet it does so, even in doses so small as only just to taste bitter. Nay, even fresh water completely blocks contraction- waves after the strip has been exposed to its influence for about half an hour, and exerts a per- manently slowing efiect after the tissue is restored to sea- water. These facts show the extreme sensi- tiveness of the neuro-muscular tissues of the Medusiie to any change in the character of their surrounding medium, a sensitiveness which we shall again have occasion to comment u})on when treating of the effects of poisons. SECTION OF COVERED-EYED MEDUSiE. 97 In conclusion, I may mention an interesting fact which is probably connected with the summation of stimuli before explained. When a contractile strip is allowed to rest for a minute or more, and when a wave is then made to traverse it, careful observation will show that the passage of the first wave is slower than that of its successor, provided the latter follows the former after not too great an interval of time. The difference, however, is exceed- ingly slight, so that to render it apparent at all the longest possible strips must be used, and even then the experimenter may fail to detect the difference, unless he has been accustomed to signalling, by which method all these observations on rate have to be made. StiinvJiis-iuaves. The rate of transmission of tentacular waves is only one-half that of contraction- waves, viz. nine inches a second. This fact appeared to me very remarkable in view of the consideration that the tentacular wave is the optical expression of a stimulus-wave, and that there can be no conceivable use in a stimulus- wave being able to pass through contractile tissue independently of a contraction- wave, unless the former is able to travel more rapidly than the latter ; for the only conceivable use of the stimulus-wave is to establish physiological harmony between different parts of the organism, and if this wave cannot travel more rapidly than a contraction-wave which starts from the same point, it would clearly fail to perform this function. 98 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. In view of this anomaly, I was led to think that if the rate of the stimulus-wave is dependent in a large degree on the strength of the stimulus that starts it, the slow rate of nine inches a second might be more than doubled, if, instead of using a stimulus so gentle as not to start a contraction- wave, I used a stimulus sufficiently strong to do this. Accordingly I chose a specimen of Aurelia wherein the occurrence of tentacular waves was very conspicuous, and found, as I. had hoped, that every time I stimulated too gently to start a con- traction-wave, the tentacular wave travelled only at the rate of nine inches a second ; whereas, if I stimulated with greater intensity, I could always observe the tentacular wave coursing an inch or two in front of the contraction-wave. It is remarkable, however, that in this, as in all the other specimens of Aurelia which I experimented upon, the reflex response of the manubrium was equally long, whatever strength of stimulus I applied to the umbrella ; or, at any rate, the time was only slightly less when a contraction- wave had passed than when only a tentacular wave had done so. The loss of time, however, appears to take place in the manubrium itself, where the rate of response is astonishingly slow. Thus, if one lobe be irritated, it is usually from four to eight seconds before the other lobes respond. But the time required for such sj^mpathetic response may be even more variable than this — the limits I have observed beino; as G:reat as from three to ten seconds. In all cases, however, the response, when it does occur, is SECTION OF COVERED-EYED MEDUSA 99 sudden, as if tlie distant lobe had then for the first time received the stimulus. Moreover, one lobe — usually one of those adjacent to the lobe directly irritated — responds before the other two, and then a variable time afterwards the latter also respond. This time is, in most cases, comparatively short, the usual limits being from a quarter of a second to two seconds. How much of these enormous intervals is occupied by the period of ganglionic latency, and how much by that of transmission, it is impossible to say; but I have determined that the rate of transmission from the end of a lobe of the manu- brium to a lithucyfc>o (deducting a second for the double period of latent stimulation) is the same as the rate of a tentacular wave, viz. nine inches a second. The presumption, therefore, is that the immense lapse of time required for reflex response on the part of the manubrium is required by the lobular ganglia, or whatever element it is that here performs the ganglionic function. Exhaustion, In various modes of section of Aurelia I have several times observed a fact that is worth record- ing. It sometimes happens that w^hen the con- necting isthmus between two almost severed areas of excitable tissue is very narrow, the passage of contraction-waves across the isthmus depends upon the freshness, or freedom from exhaustion, of the tissue which constitutes the isthmus. That is to say, on faratlizing one of the two tissue-areas which 100 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. the isthmus serves to connect, the resulting con- traction-waves will at first pa^^s freely across the isthmus ; but after a time it may happen in some preparations that every now and then a contraction- wave fails to pass across the isthmus. When this is the case, if the stimulation is still continued, a greater and greater proportion of waves fail to pass across the isthmus, until perhaps only one in every five or six becomes propagated from the one area to the other. If single induction-shocks be then sub- stituted for the faradaic stimulation, it may be found that by leaving an interval of four or five seconds between the successive shocks, every w^ave that is started in the one area will be propagated across the isthmus to the other area. But if the interval between the successive shocks be reduced to two or three seconds, every now and then a wave will fail to pass across the isthmus; and if the interval be still further reduced to one second, or half a second, comparatively few of the waves will pass across. Now, however, if the tissue be allowed five minutes' rest from stimulation, and the single shocks be thrown in at one second's intervals, all the first six or ten waves will pass across the isthmus, after which they begin to become blocked as before. It may be observed also that when the waves are thus blocked, owing to exhaustion of the connecting isthmus, they may again be made to force a passage by increasing the intensity of the stimulation, and so giving rise to stronger waves having a greater power of penetration. Thus, on re-enforcing the electrical stimulus with the simultaneous application SECTION OF COVERED-EYED MEDUSiE. 101 of a drop of spirit, the resulting waves of contraction are almost sure to pass across the isthmus, even though this has been exhausted in the manner just described. Ganglia appearing to assert their Influence at a Distance from their own Seat. Another fact, which I liave several times noticed during my sections of Aurelia, also deserves to be recorded. I have observed it under several modes of section, but it will be only necessary to describe one particular case. In the Aurelia of a portion of which the accom- panying woodcut (p. 102) is a representation, seven of the lithocysts were removed, while the remaining one was almost entirely isolated from the general contractile tissue by the incisions aa, 66, cc. The lithocyst continued to animate the tissue-area xxxx, and through tlie connecting passage y the con- traction-waves spread over the remainder of the sub-umbrella tissue zzzz. So far, of course, the facts were normal ; but very frequently it was observed that the contraction-waves did not start from the lithocyst, or from the area xxxx, but from the point o in the area zz. After this origination of the contraction-waves from the point o had been ob- served a great number of times, I removed the lithocyst. Tiie effect was not only to prevent the further origination of contraction- waves in the area xxxx, but also to prevent their further origination from the point o, the entire umbrella thus becom- 102 JELLY-FISII, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. ing paralyzed. Hence, before the removal of the lithocyst, the contraction-waves which originated at the point o, no less than those which originated at the lithocyst itself, must in some way or other have been due to the ganglionic influence emanat- ing from the lithocyst and asserting itself at the distant point o. Fig. 21. This property, which lithocysts sometimes present, of asserting their ganglionic influence at a distance from their own locality, can only, I think, be ex- plained by supposing that at the point where under these circumstances the contractions originate, there are situated some scattered gano-lionic cells of SECTION OF COVERED- EYED MEDUSAE. 103 consideraLle functional power, but yet not of power enough to originate contraction-waves unless re- enforced by some stimulating iniiuence, which reaches them from the lithocyst through the nervous plexus. Regeneration of Tissues, The only facts which remain to be stated in the present chapter have reference to the astonishing rapidity with which the excitable tissues of the Medusae regenerate themselves after injury. In this connection I have mainly experimented on Aurelia aurita, and shall, therefore, confine my remarks to this one species. If with a sharp scalpel an incision be made through the tenuous contractile sheet' of the sub- umbrella of Aurelia, in a marvellously short time the injury is repaired. Thus, for instance, if such an incision be carried across the whole diameter of the sub-umbrella, so as entirely to divide the excit- able tissues into two parts while the gelatinous tissues are left intact, the result of course is that physiological continuity is destroyed between the one half of the animal and the other, while the form of the whole animal remains unchanged — the much greater thickness of the uninjured gelatinous tissues serving to preserve the shape of the umbrella. But although the contractile sheet which lines the umbrella is thus completely severed throughout its whole diameter, it again reunites, or heals up, in from four to eight hours after the operation. CHAPTER V. EXPERIMENTS IN SECTION OF NAKED-EYED MEDUSAE. Distribution of Nerves in Sarsia. My experiments have shown that the nervous system in the naked-eyed Medusse is more highly organized, or integrated, than it is in the covered- eyed Medusae ; for whereas in the latter I obtained no evidence of the gathering together of nerve- fibres into definite bundles or trunks (the plexus being evenly distributed over the entire surface of the neuro-muscular sheet lining the umbrella), in the former I found abundant evidence of tliis advance in organization. And as the experiments in this connection serve to substantiate the histo- logical researches of Professors Haeckel, Schultz, Eimer, and Hertwig, in as far as the distribution of the main nerve-trunks is concerned, I shall here detail at some length the character and results of these experiments in the case of Sarsia. The occurrence of reflex action in Sarsia is of a very marked and unmistakable character. I may begin by stating that when any part of the internal surface of the bell is irritated, the manubrium responds; but as there is no evidence of ganglia SECTION OF NAKED-EYED MEDUSA 105 occurring in the manubrium, tliis cannot properly be regarded as a case of reflex action. But now the converse of the above statement is likewise true, viz. that when any part of the manubrium is irri- tated, the bell responds ; and it is in this that the unequivocal evidence of reflex action consists, for while the sympathy of the manubrium with the bell is not in the least impaired by removing the marginal ganglia of the latter, the sympathy of the bell with the manubrium is by this operation entirely destroyed. We have thus very excellent demonstration of the occurrence of reflex action in the Medusae. Further experiments show that the reflex action occurs, not between the marginal ganglia and every part of the manubrium, but only between the marginal ganglia and the point of the bell from which the manubrium is suspended — it being only the pull which is exerted upon this point when the manubrium contracts that acts as a stimulus to the marginal ganglia. But the high degree of sensitiveness shoAvn by the marginal ganglia to the smallest amount of traction of this kind is quite as remarkable as their lack of sensitiveness to dis- turbances going on in the manubrium. Turning now to the physiological evidence of the distribution of nerves in Sarsia, when one of the four tentacles is very gently irritated, it alone contracts. If the irritation be slightly stronger, all the four tentacles, and likewise the manubrium, contract. If one of the four tentacles be irritated still more strongly, the boll responds with one or 106 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS more locomotor contractions. If in the latter case the stimulus be not too strong, or, better still, if the specimen operated on be in a noii-vigorous or in a partly ansesthesiated state, it may be observed that a short interval elapses between ihe response of the tentacles and that of the bell. Lastl}^, the manubrium is much more sensitive to a stimulus applied to a tentacle, or to one of the marginal bodies, than it is to a stimulus applied at any other part of the nectocalyx. These facts clearly point to the inference that nervous connections unite the tentacles with one another and also with the manubrium ; or, perhaps more precisely, that each marginal body acts as a co-ordinating centre between nerves proceeding from it in four directions, viz. to the attached tentacle, to the margin on either side, and to the manubrium. This, it will be observed, is the distri- bution which Haeckel describes as occurring in Geryonia, and Schultz as occuriing in Sarsia. It is, further, the distribution to which m}^ explorations by stimulus would certainly point. But, in order to test the matter still more thoroughly, I tried the effects of section in destroying the physiological relations which I have just described. These effects, in the case of the tentacles, were sufficiently precise. A minute radial cut (only just long enough to sever the tissues of the extreme margin) introduced between each pair of adjacent marginal bodies completely destroyed the physiological connection between the tentacles. If only three marginal cuts were introduced, the sympathy between those two SECTION OF NAKliD-KYLl) MEDUSAE. 107 adjacent tentacles between wliicli no cut was made continued unini|)aired, while the sympathy between them and the other tentacles w^as destroyed. The nervous connections between the tentacles and the manubrium are of a more general character than those described between the tentacles them- selves; that is to say, severing the main radial nerve-trunks produces no appreciable effect upon the sympathy between the tentacles and the manubrium. The nervous connections between the whole excitable surface of the nectocalyx and the manu- brium are likewise of this general character, so that, whether or not the radial nerve-trunks are divided, the manubrium will respond to irritation applied anywhere over the internal surface of the nectocalyx. The manubrium, however, shows itself more sensitive to stimuli applied at some parts of this surface than it is to stimuli applied at other parts, although in different specimens there is no constancy as to the position occupied by these excitable tracts. Distribution of Kerves in Tiarojjsis Indicans. We have seen that in Sarsia reflex action obtains between the manubrium and the nectocalyx; we shall now see that in Tiaropsis indicans something resembling reflex action obtains between the nectocalyx and the manubrium. The last-named species is a new one, which I have described else- where, and I have called it " indicans " from a highly interesting and important peculiarity of 108 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS function which is manifested by its manubrium. The Medusa in question measures about one and a half inches in diameter, and is provided with a manu- brium of unusual proportional size, its length being about five-eigjhths of an inch, and its tliickness Fig. 22. being also considerable. Now, if any part of the nectocalyx be irritated, the following series of phenomena takes place. Shortly after the applica- tion of the stimulus, the large manubrium suddenly contracts — the appearance presented being that of an exceedingly rapid crouching movement. The SECTION OF NAKED-EYED MEDUSAE. 109 crouching attitude in which this movement termi- nates continues for one or two seconds, after wliich the organ begins gradually to resume its former dimensions. Concurrently with these movements on the part of the manubrium, the portion of the nectocalj^x which has been stimulated bends inwards as far as it is able. The manubrium now begins to deflect itself towards the bent-in portion of the nectocalyx ; and this deflection continuing with a somewhat rapid motion, the extremity of the manubrium is eventually brought, with unerring precision, to meet the in-bent portion of the necto- calyx. I here introduce a drawing of more than life-size to render a better idea of this "pointing action by the manubrium to a seat of irritation located in the bell. It must further be stated that in the unmutilated animal such action is quite invariable, the tapered extremity of the manubrium never failing to be placed . on the exact spot in the nectocalyx where the stimulation is being, or had previously been, applied. Moreover, if the experimenter iriitates one point of the nectocalyx, with a needle or a fine pair of forceps for instance, and while the manubrium is applied to that point he irritates another point, then the manu- brium will leave the first point and niove over to the second. In this way the manubrium may be made to indicate successively any number of points of irritation ; and it is interesting to observe that when, after such a series of irritations, the animal is left to itself, the manubrium will subsequently continue for a considerable time to visit first one no JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. and then another of the points which have been irritated. In such cases it usually dwells longest and most frequently on those points wdiich have been irritated most severely. I think the object of these movements is probably that of stinging the offending body by means of the urticating cells with which the extremity of the manubrium is armed. But, be the object what it may, the fact of these movements occurring is a highly important one in connection with our study of the distribution of nerves in Medusae, and the first point to be made out with regard to these movements is clearly as to whether or not they are truly of a reflex character. Accordingly, I first tried cuttino^ off the marg^in, and then irritatino^ the muscular tissue of the bell ; the movements in question were performed exactly as before. I was thus led to think it probable that the reflex centres of which I was in search might be seated in the manu- brium. Accordingly, I cut oft* the manubrium, and tried stimulating its own substance directly. I found, however, that no matter how small a portion of this organ I used, and no matter from wdiat part of the organ I cut it, this portion would do its best to bend over to the side which I irritated. Simi- larly, no matter how short a stump of the manu- brium I left in connection with the nectocalyx, on irritating any part of the latter, the stump of the manubrium would deflect itself towards that part of the bell, although, of course, from its short length it was unable to reach it. Hence there can be no doubt that every portion of the manubrium — down, SECTION OF NAKED-EYED MEDUSJE. Ill ab least, to the size which is compatible with con- ducting these experiments — is independently en- dowed with the capacity of very precisely localizing a point of irritation which is seated either in its own substance or in tliat of the bell. We have here, then, a curious fact, and one which it will be well to bear in mind during our subsequent endeavours to frame some sort of a con- ception regarding the nature of these primitive nervous tissues. The localizing function, which is so very efficiently performed by the manubrium of this Medusa, and which if anything resembling it occurred in the higher animals would certainly have definite ganglionic centres for its structural co-relative, is here shared equally by every part of the exceedingly tenuous contractile tissue that forms the outer surface of the organ. I am not aware that such a diffusion of ganglionic function has as j^et been actually proved to occur in the animal kingdom, but I can scarcely doubt that future investigation will show such a state of things to be of common occurrence among the lower members of that kingdom.* * The only case I know which rests on direct observation, and which is at all parallel to the one above described, is the case of the tentacles of Drosera. Mr. Darwin fonnd, when he cut ofE the apical gland of one of these tentacles, together with a small por- tion of the apex, that the tentacle thus mutilated would no longer respond to stimuli applied directly to itself. Thus far the case dillers from that of the manubrium of Tiaropsis indicans, and, in respect of localization of co-ordinating function, resembles that of ganglionic action. But Mr. Darwin also found that such a *' headless tentacle " continued to be influenced by stimuli applied to the glands of neighbouring tentacles — the headless one in that 112 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FLSH, AND SEA-URCHINS. I shall now proceed to consider the nature of the nervous connections between the nectocalj^x and manubrium of this Medusa. Bearing in mind that in an unmutilated Tiaropsis indicans the manubrium invariably localizes with the utmost precision any minute point of irrita- tion situated in the bell, the significance of the following facts is unmistakable, viz. that when a cut is introduced between the base of the manu- brium and the point of irritation in the bell, the localizing power of the former, as regards that point in the latter, is wholly destroyed. For instance, if such a cut as that represented at a (see Fig. 22) be made in the nectocalyx of this Medusa, the manu- brium will no longer be able to localize the seat of a stimulus applied below that cut, as, for instance, at h. Now, having tried this experiment a number of times, and having always obtained the same result, I conclude that the nervous connections be- tween the nectocalyx and the manubrium, which render possible the localizing action of the latter, are connections the functions of which are intensely sjDecialized, and the distribution of which is radial. So far, then, we have highly satisfactory evidence of tissue-tracts performing the function of afferent nerves. But another point of interest here arises. Although, in the experiment just described, the case bending over in whatever direction it was needful for it to bend, in order to approach the seat of stimulation. This shows that the analogue of ganglionic function must here be situated in at least more than one part of a tentacle ; and I think it is not improbable that, if trials were expressly made, this function would be found to be diffused throughout the whole tentacle. SECTION OF NAKED-EYED MEDUS.E. 113 manubrium is no longer able to localize the seat of stimulation in the bell, it nevertheless continues able to perceive, so to speak, that stimulation is being applied in the bell someiuhere ; for every time any portion of tissue below the cut a is irritated, the manubrium actively dodges about from one part of the bell to another, applying its extremity now to this place and now to that one, as if seeking in vain for the offending body. If the stimulation is persistent, the manubrium will every now and then pause for a few seconds, as if trying to decide from which direction the stimulation is proceeding, and will then suddenly move over and apply its extremity, perhaps to the point that is opposite to the one which it is endeavouring to find. It will then suddenly leave this point and try an- other, and then another, and anothei-, and so on, as long as the stimulation is continued. Moreover, it is important to observe that there are gradations between the ability of the manubrium to localize correctly and its inability to localize at all, these gradations being determined by the circum- ferential distance from the end of the cut and the point of stimulation. For instance, in Fig. 22, suppose a cut A B, quarter of an inch long, to be made pretty close to the margin and concentric with it, then a stimulus applied at the point c, just below the middle point of A B, would have the effect of making the manubrium move about to various parts of the bell, without being able in the least degree to localize the seat of irritation. But if the stimulus be applied at d, the manubrium will 114 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. probably be so far able to localize the seat of irrita- tion as to confine its movements, in its search for the offending body, to perhaps the quadrant of the bell in which the stimulation is being applied. If the stimulation be now supplied at e, the localization on the part of the manubrium will be still more accurate ; and if applied at /(that is, almost beneath one end of the cut A B), the manubrium may suc- ceed in localizing quite correctly. These facts may also be well brought out by another mode of section, viz. by cutting round a greater or less extent of the marginal tissue, leaving one end of the resulting slip free, and the other end attached in situ. If this form of section be practised on Tiaropsis indicans, as represented at g k in the figure, it may also be observed that irritation of a distant point in the marginal strip, such as g or h, causes the manubrium to move in various directions, without any special reference to that part of the bell which the irritated point of the marginal strip would occupy if in situ. But if the stimulation be applied only one or two millims. from the point of attachment of the marginal strip, as at i, the manubrium will confine its localizinof motions to perhaps the proper quadrant of the bell ; and if the stimulus be applied still nearer to the attachment of the severed strip, as at^', the localizing motions of the manubrium may become quite accurate. Asrain, wdth reirard to radial distance, if the cut A B in the figure were situated higher up in the bell, as at A' B', and the arc, c, d, e, f, of the SECTION OF NAKED-EYED MEDUSA. 115 margin irritated as before, the manubrium would be able to localize better than if, as before, the radial distance between A B and c, d, e, f were less. The greater this radial distance, the better would be the localizing power of the manubrium ; so that, for instance, if the cut A' B' were situated nearly at the base of the manubrium, the latter orofan might be able to localize correctly a stimulus ap- plied, not only as before at /, but also at e or d. In such comparative experiments, however, it is to be understood that the higher up in the bell a cut is placed, the shorter it must be ; for a fair comparison requires that the two ends of the cut shall always touch the same two radii of the nectocalyx. Still, if the cut is only a very short one (say one or two millims. long), this consideration need not practically be taken into account ; for such a cut, if situated just above the margin, as represented at a, will have the effect of destroying the localizing power of the manubrium as regards the corresponding arc of the margin; but if situated high up in the bell, even though its length be still the same, it will not have this effect. From all this, then, we have seen that the con- nections which render possible the accurate localiz- ino^ functions of the manubrium are almost, though not quite, exclusively radial. We have also seen that between accurate localization and mere random movements on the part of the manubrium there are numerous gradations, the degree of decline from one to the other depending on the topographical relations between the point of stimulation and the IIG JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISTI, AND SEA-URCHINS. end of the section (the section being of the form represented by A B in the figure). These relations, as we have seen, are the more favourable to correct localization : (a) the greater the radial distance be- tween the point of stimulation and the end of the section ; and (6) the less the circumferential distance between the point of the stimulation and the radius let fall from the end of the section. But we have seen that the limits as regards severity of section within which these gradations of localizing ability occur, are exceedingly restricted — a cut of only a few millims. in length, even though situated at the greatest radial distance possible, being sufficient to destroy all localizing power of the manubrium as regards the middle point of the corresponding arc of the margin, and a stimulus applied only a few millims. from the attached end of a severed marginal strip entirely failing to cause localizing action of the manubrium. Lastly, we have seen that even after all localizing action of the manu- brium has been completely destroyed by section of the kinds described, this organ nevertheless con- tinues actively, though inefiectually, to search for the seat of irritation. The last-mentioned fact shows that after excita- tional continuity of a higher order has been destroyed, excitational continuity of a lower order nevertheless persists ; or, to state the case in other words, the fact in question shows that after severance of the almost exclusively radial connec- tions between the bell and the manubrium, by which the perfect or unimpaired localizing function SKCTION OF NAKKD-EYED MEDUSAE 117 of the latter is rendered pussible, other coiineetions between these or^^ans remain wliich are not in any wi.se radial. 1 therefore next tested the degree in which these non-radial connections might be cut without causing destruction of that excitational continuity of a lower order which it is their func- tion to maintain. It will here suffice to record one mode of section Avhich has yielded definite results. A glance at the accompanying illustration (Fig. 23) will show the manner in which the Medusa is pre- Fig 23. pared. The margin having been removed (in order to prevent possible conduction by the marginal nerve- fibres), a single deep radial cut (a a) is first made, and then a circumferential cut (a, h, c) is carried nearly all the way round the base of the manubrium. In this way the nectocalyx, deprived of its margin, is converted into a continuous band of tissue, one of the ends of which supports the manubrium. Now it is obvious that this mode of section must be very trying to nervous connections of any kind subsist- 118 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISII, AND SEA-URCHINS. ing between the bell and the manubrium. Never- theless, in many cases, irritating any part of the band a I has the effect of causing the manubrium to perform the active random motions previously described. In such cases, however, it is observable that the further away from the manubiium the stimulus is applied, the less active is the response of this organ. In very many instances, indeed, the manubrium altogether fails to respond to stimuli applied at more than a certain distance from itself. For example, referring to Fig. 23, the manubrium might actively respond to irritation of any point in the division d, e, /, g, w^hile to irritation of any point in the division /, g, h, i its responses would be weaker, and to irritation of any point in //, i, j, k, they would be very uncertain or altogether absent. Hence in this form of section we have reached about the limit of tolerance of which the non- radial connections between the bell and manubrium are capable. Another interesting fact brought out by this form of section is, that the radial tubes are tracts of comparatively high irritability as regards the manu- brium ; for the certainty and vigour with which the manubrium responds to a stimidus aoplied at one of the severed radial tubes, /, g, or h, i, or j, k, contrast strongly with the uncertainty and feeble- ness with which it often responds to stimuli applied between any of these tubes. Indeed, it frequently happens that a specimen which will not respond at all to a stimulus applied between two radial tubes, will respond at once to a stimulus applied SECTION OF xNAKED-EVED MEDUS.E. 119 much fiirtlier from tlie manubrium, but in the course of the radial tube /A;. And til is leads us to another point of interest. In such a form of section, when any part of the mutilated nectocalyx is irritated, the manubrium shows a very marked tendency to toucli some point in the tissue-mass a a d e (Fig. 23) by which it still remains in connection with the bell, and throuirh which, therefore, the stimulus must pass in order to reach the manubrium. And it is observable that this tendency is particularly well marked if the section has been planned as represented in Fig. 23, i.e. in such a way as to leave the tissue-tract a a d e pervaded by a nutrient-tube d e, this tube being thus left intact. When this is done, the manubrium most usuall}^ points to the uninjured nutrient-tube d e every time any part of the tissue-band a Z is irritated. Let us now very briefly consider the inferences to which these results would seem to point. The fact that the localizing power of the manubrium is completely destroyed as regards all parts of the bell lying beyond an incision in the latter, con- clusively proves, as already stated, that all parts of the bell are pervaded by radial lines of differen- tiated tissue, which have at least for one of their functions the conveying of impressions to the manu- brium. The fact in question also proves that the particular effect which is produced on the manu- brium by stimulating any one of these lines cannot be so produced by stimulating any of the other lines. But althoufrh these tracts of differentiated 120 JELLY-FISH, STAK-FLSH, AND SEA-URCHINS, tissue thus far resemble afferent nerves in their function, we soon see that in one important par- ticular they differ widely from such nerves; for we have seen that, after they have been divided, stimulation of their peripheral parts still continues to be transmitted to their central parts, as shown by the non-localizing movements of the manubrium. Of course this transmission cannot take place through the divided tissue-tracts themselves; and hence the only hypothesis we can frame to account for the fact of its occurrence is that which would suppose these tissue-tracts, or afferent lines, to be capable of vicarious action. Such vicarious action would probably be efiected by means of intercom- municating fibres, the directions of w^hich would probably be various. In this way we arrive at the hypothesis of the whole contractile sheet being pervaded by an intimate plexus of functionally differentiated tissue, the constituent elements of which are capable of a vicarious action in a high degree. Now we know from histological observation that there is a plexus of nerve-fibres pervading the whole expanse of the contractile sheet, and therefore we may conclude that this is the tissue through which the effects are produced. But, if so, we must further conclude that the fibres of this nerve-plexus are capable of vicarious action in the high degree which I have explained. And this hypothesis, besides being recommended by the consideration that it is the only one avail- able, is confirmed by the fact that the stimuli which SECTION OF NAKED-EYED MEDUSiE. 121 it supposes to escape from a severed phalanx of nerve-fibres, and then to reach the manubrium after being diffused through many or all of the other radial lines (such stimuli thus converging from many directions), are responded to when they reach the manubrium, not by any decided localizing action on the part of the latter, but, as the hypothesis would lead us to expect, by the tentative and apparently random motions which are actually observed. Moreover, we must not neglect to notice that these tentative or random movements resemble in every way the localizing movements, save only in their want of precision. Again, this hypothesis is rendered more probable by the occurrence of those gradations in the localizing power of the manubrium which we have seen to be so well marked under certain conditions. The occurrence of such gradations under the conditions I have named is what the theory would lead us to expect, because the closer beneath a section that a stimulus is applied, the greater must be the immediate lateral spread of the stimulus through the plexus before it reaches the manubrium. Similarly, the further the circumferential distance from the nearest end of such a section that the stimulus is applied, the greater will be its lateral spread before reaching the manubrium. Lastly, the present hypothesis would further lead us to anticipate the fact that when Tiaropsis in- dicans is prepared as represented in Fig. 23, the manubrium refers a stimulus applied anywhere in the mutilated nectocalyx to the band of tissue by which it is still left in connection with that organ ; 122 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. for it is evident that, according to the hypothesis, the radial fibres occupying such a band are the only ones whose irritation the manubrium is able to perceive, and hence it is to be expected that it should tend to refer to these particular fibres a source of irritation occurring anywhere in the mutilated bell. It is not quite so easy to understand why, in the last-mentioned experiment, the manubrium should tend to refer a seat of irritation to the unsevered nutrient tube, or nerve-trunk, rather than to the unsevered nerves in the general nerve-plexus on either side of that nerve-trunk ; for if this nerve- trunk at all resembles in its functions the nerve- trunks of higher animals, the afferent elements collected in it ought to communicate to the manu- brium the impression of having had their distal terminations irritated, and therefore the fact of a number of such elements beincr collected into a single trunk ought not to cause the manubrium to refer a distant seat of irritation to that trunk rather than to any of the parts from which the plexus- elements may emanate. Concerning this difficulty, however, I may observe that we seem to have in it one of those cases in which it would be very un- safe to argue, with any confidence, from the highly integrated nervous systems with which we are best acquainted, to the primitive nervous systems with which we are now concerned. And although it would occupy too much space to enter into a dis- cussion of this subject, I may further observe that I think it is not at all improbable that the manu- SECTION OF NAKED EYED MEDUSA. 123 brium of Tiaropsis indicans should, in the absence of more definite information, refer a distant seat of injury to that tract of collected afferent elements through which it actually receives the strongest stimulation. Staurophoi'a Laciniata. Tliis is a Medusa about the size of a small saucer which responds to stimulation of its marginal ganglia, or radial nerve-trunks, by a peculiar spasmodic movement. This consists in a sudden and violent contraction of the entire muscle-sheet, the effect of which is to draw together all the gelatinous walls of the nectocalyx in a far more powerful manner than occurs during ordinary swim- ming. In consequence of this spasmodic action being so strong, the nectocalyx undergoes a change in form of a very marked and distinctive character. The corners of the four radial tubes, being occupied by comparatively resisting tissue, are not so much affected by the spasm as are other parts of the bell ; and they therefore constitute a sort of framework upon which the rest of the bell contracts, the whole bell thus assuming the form of an almost perfect square, with each side presenting a slight concavity inwards. These spasmodic movements, however, are quite unmistakable when they occur even in a very minute portion of detached tissue ; for, however large or small the portion may be, when in a spasm it folds upon itself with the characteris- tically strong and persistent contraction. I say 124 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. persistent contraction, becaitae a spasmodic con- traction, besides beinof of unusual strencrth, is also of unusual duration ; that is to say, while an ordinary systolic movement only lasts a short time, a spasm lasts from six to ten seconds or more, and this whether it occurs in a large or in a small piece of tissue. Ag^ain, the diastolic movements differ very much in the case of an ordinary locomotor contraction and in that of a spasm ; for while in the former case the process of relaxation is rapid even to suddenness, in the latter it is exceedingly prolonged and gradual, occupying some four or five seconds in its execution, and, from its slow but continuous nature, presenting a graceful appearance. Lastly, the difference between the two kinds of contraction is shown by the fact that, while a spasm is gradually passing off the ordinary rhythmical contractions may often be seen to be superimposed on it — both kinds of contraction being thus present in the same tissue at the same time. Now the point with which we shall be especially concerned is, that it is only stimulation of certain parts of the organism which has the effect of throw- ing it into a spasm. These parts are the margin (including the tentacles) and the courses of the four radial tubes (including the manubrium, which in this species is spread over the radial tubes). This limitation, however, is not invariable ; for I have often seen individuals of this species respond with a spasm to irritation of the general contractile tissue. Nevertheless, such response to snch stimu- lation in the case of this species is exceptional — SECTION OF NAKED-EYED MEDUSAE. 125 the usual response to muscular irritation being an ordinary locomotor contraction, which forms a marked contrast to the tonic spasm that invariably ensues upon stimulation of the margin, and almost invariably upon the stimulation of a radial tube. The first question I undertook to answer was the amount of section which the excitable tissues of Staurophora laciniata would endure without losing their power of conducting the spasmodic contraction from one of their parts to another. This was a very interesting question to settle, because Stauro- phora laciniata, like all the other species of disco- phorus naked-eyed Medusre, differs from Aurelia, etc., in that the ordinary contraction- waves are very easily blocked by section. It therefore became in- teresting to ascertain whether or not the wave of spasm admitted of being blocked as easily. First, then, as regard* the margin. If this be all cut off in a continuous strip, with the exception of one end left attached in situ, irritation of any part of the almost severed strip will cause a responsive spasm of the bell, so soon as the wave of stimulation has time to reach the latter. I next continued this form of section into the contractile tissues themselves, carrying the incision round and round the bell in the form of a spiral, as represented in the case of Aurelia by Fig. 11, page 70. In this way I con- verted the whole Medusa into a ribbon-shaped piece of tissue ; * and on now stimulating the marginal * It may be stated that while conducting this mode of section of Staurophora laciniata, the animal responds to each cut of the contractile tissues with a locomotor contraction (or it may not 126 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. tissue at one end of the ribbon, a portion of the latter would go into a spasm. The object of this experiment was to ascertain how far into the ribbon-shaped tissue the wave of spasm would penetrate. As I had expected, different specimens manifested considerable differences in this respect, but in all cases the degree of penetration was astonishingly great. For it was the exception to find cases in which the wave of spasm failed to penetrate from end to end of a spiral strip caused by a section that had been carried twice round the nectocalyx ; and this is very astonishing when we remember that the ordinary contraction-waves, whether originated by stimulation of the contractile tissues or arising spontaneously from the point of attachment of the marginal strip, usually failed to penetrate further than a quarter of the way round. Moreover, these waves of spasm will continue to penetrate such a spiral strip even after the latter has been submitted to a system of interdigitating cuts of a very severe description. Now, we have here to deal with a class of facts which physiologists will recognize as of a perfectly novel character. Why it should be that the very tenuous tracts of tissue which I have named should have the property of responding even to a feeble stimulus by issuing an impulse of a kind which throws the contractile tissues into a spasm ; why it should be that a spasm, when so originated, should respond at all) ; but each time the section crosses one of the radial tubes, the whole bell in front of the section, and the whole strip behind it, immediately go into a spasm. SECTION OF NAKED-EYED MEDUSAE. 127 manifest a power of penetration to which the normal contractions of the tissues in which it occurs bear so small a proportion ; why it is that the con- tractile tissues should be so deficient in the power of originating a spasm, even in response to the strongest stimulation applied to themselves ; — these and other questions at once suggest themselves as cpiestions of interest. At present, however, I am wholly unable to answer them ; though we may, I think, fairly assume that it is the ganglionic element in the margin, and probably also in the radial tubes, which responds to direct stimulation by discharging a peculiar impulse, which has the remarkable effect in question. I'or the sake of rendering the matter quite clear, let us employ a somewhat far-fetched but convenient metaphor. We may compare the general contractile tissues of this Medusa to a mass of gun-cotton, which responds to ignition (direct stimulation) by burning with a quiet flame, but to detonation (marginal stimulation) with an explosion. In the tissue, as in the cotton, every fibre appears to be endowed with the capacity of liberating energy in either of two very different ways ; and whenever one part of the mass is made, by the ap- propriate stimulus, to liberate its energy in one of these two ways, all other parts of the mass do the same, and this no matter how far through the mass the liberating process may have to extend. Now, employing this metaphor, what we find is that, while the contractile fibres resemble the cotton fibres in the respects just mentioned, the ganglion cells resemble detonators, when themselves directly 128 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. stimulated. In other words, the ganglion-cells of this Medusa are able to originate two very different kinds of impulse, according as they liberate their energy spontaneously or in answer to direct stimu- lation, and the muscular tissues respond with a totally ditferent kind of contraction in the two cases. Possibly, indeed, direct stimulation of the ganglia is followed by a spasm of the muscular tissue only because a greater amount of ordinary ganglion intiuence is thus liberated than in the case of a merely spontaneous discharge. If this were the explanation, however, I should not expect so great a contrast as there is between the facility with which a spasm may be caused by stimulation of the margin and of the contractile tissue respec- tively. The slightest nip of the margin of Stauro- phora laciniata, for instance, is sufficient to cause a spasm, whereas even crushing the contractile tissues with a large pair of dissecting-forceps Avill probably fail to cause anything other than an ordinary contraction. Nevertheless, pricking the margin with a fine needle usually has the effect of causing only a locomotor contraction. In conclusion, I may state that amesthetics have the effect of blocking the spasmodic wave in any portion of tissue that is submitted to their in- fluence. It is always observable, however, that this effect is not produced till after spontaneity has been fully suspended, and even muscular irritability destroyed as regards direct stimulation. Up to this stage the certainty and vigour of the spasm consequent on marginal irritation are not per- SECTION OF NAKED-EYED MEDUSAE. 129 ceptibly impaired ; but soon after this stage the intensity of the spasm begins to become less, and later still it assumes a local character. It is im- portant, also, to notice that at this stage the effect of marginal stimulation is ver}^ often that of pro- ducing a general locomotor contraction, and some- times a series of two or three such. During recovery in normal sea-water all these phases occur in reverse order. CHAPTER VI. CO-ORDINATION. Covered-eyed MeduscB. From the fact that in tlie covered-eyed Medussethe passage of a stimulus-wave is not more rapid than that of a contraction-wave, we may be prepared to expect that in these animals the action of the locomotor ganglia is not, in any proper sense of the term, a co-ordinated action ; for if a stimulus- wave cannot outrun a contraction-wave, one ganglion cannot know that another ganglion has discharged its influence till the contraction-wave, which re- sults from a dischar^je of the active o^anHion, has reached the passive one. And this I find to be generally the case ; for it may usually be observed that one or more of the lithocysts are either tempo- rarily or permanently prepotent over the others, i.e. that contraction-waves emanate from the pre- potent lithocysts, and then spread rapidly over the swimming-bell, without there being any signs of co-ordinated or simultaneous action on the part of the other lithocysts. Nevertheless, in many cases such prepotency cannot, even with the greatest care, be observed; but upon every pulsation all CO-ORDINATION. 131 parts of the swimming-bell seem to contract at the same instant. And this apparently perfect co- ordination amono- the eiii'ht marginal o-ano'lia mav O O O O O V continue for any length of time. I believe, however, that such apparently complete physiological har- mony is not co-ordination properly so called, i.e. is not due to special nervous connections between the ganglia ; for, if such were the case, perfectly syn- chronous action of this kind ought to be the rule rather than the exception. I am therefore inclined to account for these cases of perfectly synchronous action by supposing that all, or most, of the ganglia require exactly the same time for their nutrition ; that they are, further, of exactly equal potency in relation to the resistance (or excitability) of the surrounding contractile tissues ; and that, therefore, the balance of forces being exactly equal in the case of all, or most, of the ganglia, their rhythm, though perfectly identical, is really independent. I confess, however, that I am by no means certain regarding the accuracy of this conclusion, as it is founded on negative rather than on positive considerations : that is to say, I arrive at this conclusion regarding the cases in which such apparent co-ordination is observable only because in other cases such apparent co-ordination is not observable ; and also, I may add, because my experiments in section have not revealed any evidence of nervous connections capable of con- ducting a stimulus-wave with greater rapidity than a contraction-wave. I therefore consider this con- clusion an uncertain one, and its uncertainty is, 132 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. perhaps, still further increased by the result of the following experiments. If a covered-eyed Medusa be chosen in which perfectly synchronous action of the ganglia is o))servable, and if a deep radial incision be made between each pair of adjacent ganglia — the incisions beinof thus eiijht in number and carried either from the margin towards the centre or vice versa — it then becomes conspicuous enough that the eight partially divided segments no longer present syn- chronous action ; for now one segment and now another takes the initiative in starting a contraction- wave, which is then propagated to the other seofments. And it is evident that this fact tends to negative the above explanation, for if the discharges of the ganglia are independently simultaneous before section, we might expect them to continue so after section. It must be remembered, however, that the form of section we are considering is a severe one, and that it must therefore not only give rise to general shock, but also greatly interfere with the passage of contraction-waves, and, in general, disturb the delicate conditions on which, according to the suggested explanation, the previous harmony depended. Besides, as we shall subsequently see, for some reason or other segmentation of a Medusa profoundly modifies the rate of its rhythm. In view of these considerations, therefore, the results yielded by such experiments must not be regarded as having any conclusive bearing on the question before us ; and as these or similar objections apply to various other modes of section by which CO-OllDINATION. 138 I have endeavoured to settle this question, I will not here occupy space in detailing them. It seems desirable, however, in this connection again to mention a fact briefly stated in a former chapter, namely, that section conclusively proves a contraction-wave to have the power, when it reaches a lithocyst, of stimulating the latter into activity ; for it is not difficult to obtain a series of lithocysts connected in such a manner that the resistance offered to the passage of the waves by a certain width of the junction-tissue, is such as just to allow the residuum of the contraction- wave which emanates from one lithocyst to reach the adjacent lithocyst, thus causing it to originate another wave, wdiich, in turn, is just able to pass to the next lithocyst in the series, and so on, each lithocyst in turn acting like a reinforcing battery to the passage of the contraction- wave. Now this fact, I think, sufficiently explains the mechanism of ganglionic action in those cases where one or more lithocysts are prepotent over the others ; that is to say, the prepotent lithocyst first originates a con- traction-wave, which is then successively reinforced by all the other lithocysts during its passage round the swimming-bell. In this way the passage of a contraction-wave is no doubt somewhat accele- rated ; for I found, in marginal strips, that the rate of transit from a terminal lithocyst to the other end of the strip was somewhat lowered by excising the seven intermediate lithocysts. I may here state, in passing, a point of some little interest in connection with this reinforcing 10 134 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. action of litliocjsts. When I first observed this action, it appeared to me a mysterious thing why its result was alwa^^s to propagate the contraction- wave in only one direction — the direction, namely, in which the wave happened to be ]mssing before it reached the lithocyst. For instance, suppose we have a strip A D, Avith a lithocyst at each of the equidistant points A, B, C, D ; and suppose that the lithocyst B originates a stimulus : the resulting contraction- wave passes, of course, with equal rapidity in the two opposite directions, B A, B C (arrov/s h a, h c) : the contraction- wave h a therefore F'-. 24. reaches the lithocyst A at the snme time as the contraction-wave b c reaches the lithocyst C, and so both A and C discharge simultaneously. What, then, should we expect to be the result ? I think we should expect the wave b c to continue on its course to D, alter having been strengthened at C, and a reflex wave a' V to start from A (owing to the discharge at A), which would reach B at the same time as a similar reflex wave c' V starting from C (owing to the discharge at C) ; so that by the time the original wave b c d had reached D, the point B would be the seat of a collision between the two reflex waves a' b' and c' l_v(lia(lc'iii!ita olii::()l)l(»i » 38 0 n « 33 0 » « 33 0 This rate continued quite regularly for a quarter of an hour, when the observation terminated. It might naturally be supposed that when the alterations of temperature between 34° and 49° produce such marked effects on the rhythm, still 1C8 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. greater alterations would be attended with still greater effects. Such, however, is not the case. Water at 70° or 80°, for instance, has the effect of permanently diminisJihig the rate of the rhythm, after having temporarily raised it for a few seconds. The following experiment will serve to convey a just estimation of these facts. An Aurelia whose rhythm in water at 40° was very regular at eighteen per minute, was suddenly transferred to water at 80°. In the immediately succeeding minutes the rhythm was 22, 20, 14. The latter rate continued for nearly half an hour, when the observation terminated. The effect of very warm water, therefore, is to slow the rhythm, as well, I may add, as to enfeeble the vio'our of the contractions. The case of Medusie thus differs, in the former respect, from that of the heart; and I think the reason of the difference is to be found in the following considerations. Even slight elevations of temperature are quickly fatal to the Med usee, so it becomes presumable that con- siderable elevations act very destructively on the neuro-muscular tissues of those animals. This de- structive effect of high temperatures may, therefore, very probably counteract tlie stimulating effect which such temperatures would otherwise exert on the natural rhythm, and hence a point would somewhere be reached at which the destructive effect would so far overcome the stimulating effect as to slow the rhythm. That this is probably the true, as it cer- tainly is the only explanation to be rendered, will, I think, be conceded when I further state that if NATURAL RHYTHM. 169 an Aurclia be loft for some little time in water at 80°, and then again transferred to water at 30° or 40°, its original rate of rhythm at the latter tem- perature does not again return, bat the rhythm remains permanently slowed. And, in favour of the explanation just offered, it may be further pointed out that the first effect of sudden immer- sion in heated water is to quicken the rhythm, it not being for a few seconds, or for even a minute or two after the immersion, that the rhythm be- comes slowed. Lastly, the slowing takes place gradually ; and this is what we should expect if, as is probable, the destructive eftect takes some^vhat more time to become fully developed than does the stimulating effect. Before leaving the subject of temperature in relation to rhythm, I must say a few words on the effects of cold. The following may be regarded as typical experiments. An Aurelia presenting a regular rhythm of twenty per minute in water at 45° was placed in water at 19°. Soon after the transference the rhythm began to slow, and the strength of the contractions to diminish. Both these phenomena rapidly became more and more pronounced, till the rhythm fell to ten per minute (still quite regular), and the contractions ceased to penetrate the muscular tissue further than an inch or so from the marginal ganglia. Shortly after this stage pauses became frequent, but mechanical or other irritation always originated a fresh swimming bout. Next, only one very feeble contraction was given at long and 170 jELLY-nsn, star-fish, and sea-urchins. irregular intervals, a contraction so feeble that it was restricted to the immediate vicinity of the lithocyst in which it originated. Soon after this stage irritability towards all kinds of stimuli entirely ceased, including even strong spirit dropped on the under surface of the animal when taken momentarily out of the water. All these stages thus described were passed through rapidly, the whole series occu- pying rather less than five minutes. On now leav- ing the specimen for ten minutes and then restoring it to its original water at 45°, all the above-men- tioned stages were passed through in reverse order. The first faint marginal contraction was confined to the immediate vicinity of the prepotent lithocyst, and all subsequent contractions continued to be so for the next three minutes. Rhythm very slow. Contractions now began to penetrate round the margin, and in eight minutes from the restoration had gone all the way round, the rate of their rhythm meanwhile increasing. In two minutes more all the umbrella was contracting at the rate of fifteen per minute. In another specimen, subjected to the same con- ditions, the rate of recovery was even more rapid, occupying only two minutes altogether ; but in every case the process of recovery is a gradual one, and differs only in the time it occupies in passing through the various stages. Effects of Freezing Meduscs. In conclusion, I will describe some rather interest- ing experiments that consisted in freezing some NATUKAL RHYTHM. 171 specimens of Aurelia into a solid block of ice. Of course, as sea-water had to be employed, the cold required was very considerable ; but I succeeded in turning out the Medusae encased on all sides in a continuous block of sea- water. By now immersing this block in w^arm water, I was able to release the contained specimens, which then presented a very extraordinary appearance. The thick and massive gelatinous bell of a Medusa is, as every one knows, chiefly composed of sea-water, which everywhere enters very intimately into the structure of the tissue. Now, all this sea-water was, of course, frozen in situ, so that the animals were every- where and in all directions pierced through by an innumerable multitude of ice-crystals, which formed a very beautiful meshwork, pervading the whole substance of their transparent tissues. These experiments were made in order to ascertain whether the Medusae, after having been thus com- pletely frozen, would survive on being again thawed out, and, if so, whether the freezing process would exert any permanent influence on the rate of their rhythm. Now in all the cases the Medusae, after having been thawed out, presented a ragged ap- pearance, which was due to the disintegrating eflfect exerted by the ice-crystals while forming in the tis^es ; yet notwithstanding this mechanical injury superimposed on the physiological effects of such extreme cold, all the Medusre recovered on being restored to sea-water of the normal tem- perature. The time occupied by the process of recover}^ varied in different individuals from a few 172 JELLY-FISH, STAR- FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. minutes to half an hour or more, and it was observ- able that those specimens which recovered soonest had the rate of their rhythm least affected by the freezino-. In no case, however, that I observed did tlie rate of the rhythm after the freezing return fully to that which had been manifested before the freezing. Effects of Certain Gases on the Rhythn. Oxygen. — I will now conclude my remarks on rhythm by very briefly describing the effects of certain gases. Oxygen forced under pressure into sea- water containing Sarsire has the effect of greatly accelerating the rate of their rhythm. The follow- ing observation on a single specimen will serve to render this apparent. Number of pulsations given by Sarsia in succes- sive five-minute intervals. In ordinary sea-water . . . 472, 527, 470 In oxygenated sea-water . . . 800 ]u ordinary sea- water . . . 268, 350, 430 It will be seen from this observation that the acceleration of the rhythm due to the oxygenation was most marked ; indeed, the pulsations followed one another so rapidly that it was no easy matter to count them. It must also be stated that while the animal was under the influence of oxygen, the duration of the natural pauses between the swim- ming bouts was greatly curtailed, the swimming motions, in fact, being almost quite continuous throuo-hout the five minutes tliat the Medusa was NATURAL RHYTHM. 173 exposed to such influence. Lastly, it will be ob- served from the above table that the unnatural amount of activity displayed by the organism while in the oxygenated water entailed on it a consider- able degree of exhaustion, as shown by the fact that even a quarter of an hour after its restoration to normal water its original degree of energy had not quite returned. Carbonic acid. — As might be expected, this gas has the opposite effects to those of oxygen. It is therefore needless to say more about this agent, except that if administered in large doses it destroys both spontaneity and irritability. Nevertheless, if its action is not allowed to last too long, the Medusae will fully recover on being again restored to normal sea-water. Nitrous oxide. — This gas at first accelerates the motions of Sarsia, but eventually retards them. I omitted, however, to push the experiment to the stage of complete anaesthesia, which would doubt- less have supervened had the pressure of the gas been sufficiently great. Deficient aeration. — It may now be stated that the Medusae are exceedingly sensitive to such slight carbonization of the water in which they are con- tained as results from their being confined in a limited body of it for a few hours. The rhythm becomes slowed and the contractions feeble, while the pauses between the swimming bouts become more frequent and prolonged. If the water is not changed, all these sym[)toms become more marked, and, in addition, the rhythm becomes very irregular. 174 JELLY-FJSH, STAll-FlSlI, AND SEA-URCHINS. Eventually the swimming motions entirely cease ; but almost immediately after the animals are restored to normal sea-water, they recover them- selves completely, the rate and regularity of their rhythm being then quite natural. The suddenness with which this return to the normal state of things is effected cannot but strike the observer as very remarkable, and I may menti(m that it takes place with equal suddenness at whatever stage in the above-described process of asphyxiation the transference to normal sea- water is accomplished. CHAPTER VIII. ARTIFICIAL RHYTHM. If the umbrella of Aurelia aurita has been para- lyzed by the removal of its lithocysts, and if it is then subjected to faradaic stimulation of minimal intensity, the response which it gives is not tetanic, but rhythmic. The rate of this artificial rhythm varies in different specimens, but the limits of variation are always within those which are ob- served by the natural rhythm of diff*erent specimens. The artificial rhythm is not in every case strictly regular; but by carefully adjusting the strength of the current, and by shifting the electrodes from one part of the tissue to another until the most appro- priate part is ascertained, the artificial rhythm admits in most cases of being rendered tolerably regular, and in many cases as strictly regular as is the natural rhythm of the animal. To show this, I append a tracing of the artificial rhythm (Fig. 25), which may be taken as a fair sample of the most perfect regularity that can be obtained by minimal faradaic stimulation.* * This and all the subscqujut tracings I obtained by the method already described. ARTIFICIAL RHYTHM. 177 This artificial rhythm may be obtained with a portion of in-itable tissue of any size, and whether a large or small piece of the tissue employed be included between the electrodes. As the fact of this wonderfully rhythmic response to faradaic irritation was quite unexpected by me, and as it seemed to be a fact of great significance, I was led to investigate it in as many of its bearings as time permitted. First, I tried the etfect on the rhythm of progressively intensifying the strength of the faradaic current. I found that with each increment of the current the rate of the rhythm was increased, and this up to the point at which the rhythm began to pass into tetanus due to sum- mation of the successive contractions. But between the slowest rhythm obtainable by minimal stimula- tion and the most rapid rhythm obtainable before the appearance of tetanus, there were numerous degrees of rate to be observed. I here append another tracing, to show the effect on the rate of the rhythm of alterations in the strength of the current (Fig. 20). It will also be observed from this tracing that, in consequence of the current having been strengthened slightly beyond the limit within which strictly rhythmic response was attainable, the curves in the middle part of the tracing, where the current was strengthened, are slightly irregular. This irregu- larity is, of course, due to the first appearance of tumultuous tetanus. If the faradaic stimulation had in this case been progressively made still stronger, the irre;rularity would have become still 178 JELLr-FlSII, STAR-FISn, AND SEA-URCHINS. more pronounced up to a certain point, when it would gradually have begun to pass into more persistent tetanus. But as in this case, instead of streno^thenino^ the current still further, I as^ain weakened it to its original intensity, the rhythm immediately returned to its original rate and regu- larity. Such being the facts, the question arises as to their interpretation. At first I was naturally in- clined to suppose that the artificial rhythm was due to a periodic variation in the strength of the stimulus, caused by some slight breach of contact between the terminals and the tissue on each con- traction of the latter. This supposition, of course, would divest the phenomena in question of all physiological meaning, and I therefore took pains in the first instance to exclude it. This I did in two ways : first, by observing that in many cases (and especially in Cyanyea capillata) the rate of the rhythm is so slow that the contractions do not follow one another till a considerable interval of total relaxation has intervened; and second, by placing the terminals close together, so as to include only a small piece of tissue between, and then firmly pinning the tissue all round the electrodes to a piece of wood placed beneath the Medusa. In this way the small portion of tissue which served as the seat of stimulation was itself prevented from moving, and therefore the rhythmic motions which the rest of the Medusa presented cannot have been due to any variations in the quality of the contact between the electrodes and this stationary scat of stimulation. A?.TIFICIAL RHYTHM. 179 Any such merely mechanical source of fallacj" being thus, I think, excluded, we are compelled to regard the facts of artificial rhythm as of a purely physiological kind. The question, therefore, as to the explanation of these facts becomes one of the liighest interest, and the hypothesis which I have framed to answer it is as follows. Every time the tissue contracts it must as a consequence suffer a certain amount of exhaustion, and therefore must become slightly less sensitive to stimulation than it was before. After a time, however, the ex- haustion will pass away, and the original degree of sensitiveness will thereupon return. Noav, the intensity of faradaic stimulation which is alone capable of producing rhythmic response, is either minimal or but slightly more than minimal in relation to the sensitiveness of the tissue when fresh ; consequently, when this sensitiveness is somewhat lowered by temporary exhaustion, the intensity of the stimulation becomes somewhat less than minimal in relation to this lower degree of sensitiveness. The tissue, therefore, fails to perceive the presence of the stimulus, and consequently fails to respond. But so soon as the exhaustion is com- pletely recovered from, so soon will the tissue again perceive the presence of the stimulus ; it will there- ibre again respond, again become temporarily ex- hausted, again fail to perceive the presence of the stimulus, and again become temporarily quiescent. Now it is obviou.i tluit if this process occurs once, it may occur an iudoiinite number of times; and as the conditions of nutrition, as well as those of 180 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-UllCHINS. stimulation, remain constant, it is manifest that the responses may thus become periodic. In order to test the truth of this hypothesis, I made the following experiments. Having first noted the rate of the rhj^thm under faradaic stimu- lation of minimal intensity, without shifting the electrodes or altering the intensity of the current, I discarded the faradaic stimulation, and substituted for it single induction shocks thrown in with a key. I found, as I had hoped, that the maximiimii num- ber of these single shocks which I could thus throw in in a given time so as to procure a response to every shock, corresponded with the number of con- tractions which the tissue had previously given during a similar interval of time when under the influence of the faradaic current of similar intensity. To make this quite clear, I shall describe the whole course of one such experiment. The degaiiglionated tissue under the influence of minimal faradaic stimu- lation manifested a perfectly regular rhythm of thirty contractions per minute, or one contraction in every two seconds. While the position of the platinum electrodes and the intensity of the curi-ent lemained unchancfed, single induction shocks were now administered with a key at any intervals which might be desired. It was found that if these single induction stimuli were administered at regular intervals of two seconds or more, the tissue re- sponded to every stimulus; while if the stimuli were thrown in more rapidly than this, the tissue did not respond to every stimulus, but only to those that were separated from one another by an interval ARTIFICIAL RHYTHM. 181 of at least two seconds' duration. Thus, for in- stance, if the shocks were thrown in at the rate of one a second, the tissue only, but always, responded to every alternate shock. And similarly, as just stated, if any number of shocks were thrown in, the tissue only responded once in every two seconds. Now, as this rate of response precisely coincided with the rate of rhythm previously shown by the same tissue under the influence of faradaic stimula- tion of the same intensity, the experiment tended to verify the hypothesis which it was designed to test. I may give one other experiment having the same object and tendency. Employing single induction shocks of slightly more than minimal intensity, and throwing them in at twice the rate that was required to produce a strong response to every shock, I found that midway between every two strong responses there was a weak response. In other words, a stimulus of uniform intensity gives rise alternately to a strong and to a weak contrac- tion, as shown in the appended tracing (Fig. 27). It will be observed that in this tracing^ each lar2:e curve represents the whole time occupied by the strong contraction, the latter beginning at the , highest point of the curve on the left-hand side in each case. The eiFect of the weak contraction is that of momentarily interrupting the even sweep of diastole after the strong contraction, and there- fore the result on the tracing is a slight depression in the otherwise even curve of ascent. Lest any doubt should arise from the smallness of the curves representing the weak contractions that the former 13 ARTIFICIAL RHYTHM. 183 are in some way accidental, I may draw attention to the fact that the period of latent stimulation is the same in the case of all the curves. To render this apparent, I have placed crosses below the smaller curves, which show in each case the exact point where the depressing effect of these smaller curves on the ascending sweeps of the larger curves first become apparent — i.e. the point at which the feeble contraction begins. Now, what I wish to be gathered from the whole tracing is this. If the strenofth of the induction shocks had been much greater than it was, all the contractions would have become strong contractions, and tetanus would have been the result. But, as the strength of the in- duction shocks was only slightly more than minimal, the exhaustion consequent on every strong con- traction so far diminished the irritability of the tissue that when, during the process of relaxation, another shock of the same intensity was thrown in, the stimulus was only strong enough, in relation to the diminished irritability of the partly recovered tissue, to cause a feeble contraction. And these facts tend still further to substantiate the hypothesis whereby I have sought to explain the phenomena of artificial rhythm. Now, I think that the strictly rhythmic action of the paralyzed swimming-bell of Aurelia in answer to constant stimulation is a fact of the highest significance ; for here we have a tissue wholly, or almost wholly, deprived of its centres of spontaneity, yet pulsating as rhythmically in answer to artificial stimulation as it previously did 184 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. in answer to ganolionic stimulation.* Does not O CD this tend to show that for the production of the natural rhythm the presence of the ganglionic element is non-essential ; that if we merely suppose the function of this element to be that of supplying a constant stimulus of a low intensity, without in addition supposing the presence of any special resistance-mechanism to regulate the discharges, the periodic sequence of systole and diastole would assuredly result ; and, therefore, that the rhythmical character of the natural swimming motions is dependent, not on the peculiar relations of the ganglionic, but on the primary qualities of the contractile tissue ? Or, if we do not go so far as this (and, as I may parentis etically observe, I am not myself inclined to go so far), must we not at least conclude that the natural rhythm of these tissues is not exchisively due to any mechanism whereby the discharges of the ganglia are inter- rupted at regular intervals; but that whether these discharges are supposed to be interrupted or continuous, the natural rhythm is probably in a large measure due to the same cause as the artificial rliythm, viz. in accordance with our previous hypothesis, to the alternate exhaustion and recovery of the excitable tissues ? This much, at least, must be allowed even by the most cautious * It will not be forgotten that there are a mnltitnde of ganglion-cells distributed throughout the contractile tissues of the Medusae ; but forasmuch as these are comparatively rarely- instrumental in originating stimulation, I think it is probable that artificial stimulation acts directly on the contractile tissues, and not through the medium of these scattered cells. ARTIFICIAL RHYTHM. 185 of critics, viz. that if, as current views respecting the theory of rhythm would suppose, it is ex- clusively the ganglionic element which in the unmutilated Aurelia causes the rhythm of the swimming motions by intermittent stimulation, surely it becomes a most unexpected and unaccount- able fact, that after the removal of this element the contractile tissues should still persist in their display of rhythm under the influence of constant stimulation. At any rate no one, I think, will dispute that the facts which I have adduced justify us in reconsidering the whole theory of ihythm as due to ganglia. As I have already said, I am not inclined to deny that there is probably some truth in the current theory of rhythm as due to ganglia; 1 merely wish to point out distinctly that this theory is inadequate, and that in order to cover all the facts it will require to be supplemented by the theory which I now propose. The current theory of rhythm as due to ganglia attributes the whole of the effect to the ganglionic element, and thus fails to meet the fact of a rhythm which is artifi- cially produced after the ganglionic element has been removed. It also fails to meet a number of other facts of the first importance ; for it is be- yond all doubt that rhythmic action of the strictest kind occurs in an innumerable multitude of cases where it is quite impossible to suppose anything resembling ganglia to be present. Not to mention such cases as the Snail's heart, where the most careful scrutiny has failed to detect the least ves- 186 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. tige of ganglia, but to descend at once to the lowest forms of animal and vegetable life, rhythmic action may here be said to be the rule rather than the exception. The beautifully regular motions observable in some Alg?e, Diatomacese, and Ocilla- torise, in countless numbers of Infusoria, Anthero- zoids, and Spermatozoa, in ciliary action, and even in the petioles of Hedysarum gyrano, are all in- stances (to which many others might be added) of rhythmical action where the presence of ganglia is out of the question. Again, in a general way, is it not just as we recede from these primitive forms of contractile tissue that we find rhythmic action to become less usual ? And, if this is so, may it not be that those contractile tissues which in the higher animals manifest rhythmic action are the contractile tissues which have longest retained their primitive endowment of rhythmicality ? To my mind it seems hard to decide in what respect the beating of a Snail's heart differs from that of the pulsatile vesicles of the Infusoria; and I do not think it would be much easier to decide in what essential respect it differs from the beating of the Mammalian heart. The mere fact that the presence of ganglia can be proved in the one case and not in the other, seems to me scarcely to justify the conclusion that the rhythm is in the one case wholly dependent, and in the other as wholly in- dependent, of the ganglia. At any rate, this fact, if it is a fact, is not of so self-evident a character as to recommend to us the current theory of gang- lionic action on d priori grounds. ARTIFICIAL IllIYTHM. 187 Coming, then, to experimental tests, we have already seen that in the deganglionated swimming organ of Anrclia aiirita, rhythmic response is yielded to constant faradaic stimulation of low intensity. The next question, therefore, which presents itself in relation to our subject is as to whether other modes of constant stimulation elicit a similar re- sponse. Now, in a general way, I may say that such is the case, although I have chosen faradaic stimulation for special mention, because, in the first place, its effect in producing rhythmic action is the most certain and precise; and, in the next place, the effects of administering instantaneous shocks at given intervals admit of being compared with the effects of constant faradaic stimulation better than with any other kind of constant stimulation. Nevertheless, as just stated, other modes of con- stant stimulation certainly have a more or less marked effect in producing rhythmic response. The constant current, during the whole time of its passage, frequently has this effect in the case of the paralyzed nectocalyx of Sarsia ; and dilute spirit, or other irritant, when dropped on the paralyzed swimming organ of Aurelia aurita, often gives rise to a whole series of rhythmical pulsations, the sys- toles and diastoles following one another at about the same rate as is observable in the normal swim- ming motions of the unmutilated animal. From this it will be seen that, both in the case of mechanical and of chemical stimulation, the same tendency to the production of rhythmic response on the part of the paralyzed tissues of Aurelia may 188 JELLY-FISir, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. be observed as in the case of electrical stimulation. The principal differences consist in the rhythm being much less sustained in the former than in the latter case. But, by experimenting on other species of Medusae, I have been able to obtain, in response to mechanical and chemical stimula- tion, artificial rhythm of a much more sustained character than that which, under such modes of stimulation, occurs in Aurelia. I have no explana- tion to offer why it is that some species, or some tissues, present so much more readiness to manifest sustained rhythm under certain modes of stimula- tion, and less readiness to manifest it under other modes, than do other species or tissues. Probably these differences depend on some peculiarities in the irritability of the tissues which it is hopeless to ascertain; but, in any case, the facts remain, that while Aurelia, Cyanaea, and the covered-eyed Medusae generally are the best species for obtaining artificial rhythm under the influence of faradaic stimulation, some of the naked-eyed Medusae are the best species for obtaining it under the influence of the constant current, and also under that of mechanical and chemical stimulation. I have already spoken of this effect of the constant cur- rent in the case of Sarsia; I shall now proceed to describe the effects of mechanical and chemical stimulation on the same species. It is but rarely that artificial rhythm can be pro- duced in the paralyzed nectocalyx of Sarsia by means of mechanical stimulation, but in the case of the manubrium, a very decided, peculiar, and ARTIFICIAL RHYTHM. 189 persistent rh^^tlim admits of being produced by this means. lu this particular species, the manu- brium never exhibits any spontaneous motion after the ganglia of the nectocalyx have been removed. But if it be nijiped with the forceps, or otherwise irritated, it contracts strongly and suddenly ; it then very slowly and gradually relaxes until it has regained its original k-ngth. After a considerable interval, and without the application of any addi- tional stimulus, it gives another single, sudden, though slight contraction, to be again followed by gradual relaxation and a prolonged interval of repose, which is followed in turn by another con- traction, and so on. These sudden and well-marked contractions occur at intervals of many seconds, and show a decided tendency to rhythmic periodi- city, though the rhythm is not always perfectly exact. This intensely slow rhythm, as the result of injury, may continue for a long time, particu- larly if the injury has been of a severe character. There can be no doubt, therefore, that the mecha- nical (or other) injury in this case acts as a source of constant irritation ; so that here again we have evidence of rhythmic action independent of ganglia, and caused by the alternate exhaustion and re- covery of contractile tissues.* * We may pretty safely conclude that ganglia are altogether absent in the manubrium of Sarsia, not only because Schultz has failed to detect them in this organ microscopically, but also because of the complete absence of spontaneity which it mani- fests. I may here mention that this case of the manubrium of Sarsia is precisely analogous to another which I have observed in a widely different tissue, namely, the tongue of the frog. Here, 190 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-UECHINS. With regard to artificial rhythm caused by- chemical stimuli, by far the most conspicuous instance that I have observed is that of the para- lyzed nectocalyx of Sarsia. This consists in a highly peculiar motion of a flurried, shivering character, which is manifested by this organ when its marginal ganglia have been removed and it is exposed to the influence of faintly acidulated water. Now, when read in the light of the foregoing facts, there can be no doubt that the present one falls into its place very satisfactorily : it is an additional and very valuable instance of the display of arti- ficial rhythm under the influence of a constant stimulus of low intensity ; for the shivering motions of the mutilated nectocalyx under these circumstances are most unmistakably of a rhythmic nature. Viewed from a little distance, indeed, these motions are not distinguishable from the natural swimming motions of the unmutilated animal, except that, not being of quite such a powerful character, they are not so eftective for locomotion. Viewed more closely, however, it may frequently be seen that the whole bell does not contract simul- taneously, but that, as it were, clouds of contraction pass now over one part and now over another. Still, whether the contractions are partial or uni- versal, they are more or less rhythmical. As this was the only case that had ever been observed of too, the presence of ganglion-cells has never been observed micro- scopically, though specially souglit for by Dr. Klein and others. Yet, under the influence of mechanical and other modes of stimu- lation, I find that I am able to make the excised organ pulsate as rhythmically as a heart. ARTIFICIAL RHYTHM. 191 rhythm as due to a constant chemical stimulus, I studied it with much care, and shall now give a full description of what appears to me an im- portant body of physiological facts. Ten to twenty drops of acetic acid having been added to one thousand cubic centimetres of sea- water, and the paralyzed bell of Sarsia having been placed in the mixture, an interval of about half a minute will elapse before any movement begins. Sooner or later, however, the artificial rhythm is 6ure to be induced, and it will then continue for a variable time — occasionally as long as an hour, and generally for a considerable number of minutes. After it ceases it may often be made to recommence, either by adding a few more drops of acid to the sea-water, or by supplying an additional stimulus to the bell by nipping it with the forceps. Even- tually, however, all movement ceases, owing to the destruction of irritability by the action of the acid. By this time the whole inner surface of the bell has become strongly opalescent, owing to the destruc- tive influence of the acid on the epithelial cells which overspread the irritable tissues. The latter fact is worth men'-ioning, because in no case does the artificial rhythm set in until this opalescence has begun to show itself ; and as this opalescence is but an optical expression of the damage which the epithelial coat is undergoing, the explanation of the time which elapses after the first immersion of the bell in the acidulated water and the com- mencement of the artificial rhythm no doubt is, that duri])g this time the acid has not obtained 192 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. sufficient access to the excitable tissues to serve as an adequate stimulus. During the soaking stage of the ex|)eriment — i.e. before the artificial rhythm begins — the excitability of the tissues may be observed progressively and abnormally to increase ; for soon after the soaking stage begins, in response to a single nip with the forceps the bell may give two or three locomotor contractions, instead of a single one, as is invariably the case with a paralyzed bell of Sarsia in normal water. Later on during the soaking stage, four or five successive contractions may be yielded in response to a single mechanical stimulus, and shortly after this a whole bout of rhythmic contractions may be started by the same means. Indeed, in some cases the artificial rhythm in acidulated water requires such a single additional stimulus for its inauguration, the shivering movements failing to begin spontaneously, but beginning immediately upon the application of the additional stimulus. Similarly, after the shivering movements have ceased, a fresh bout may very often be started by again giving the motionless nectocalyx a single stimulation. The interpretation of these facts would seem to be that the general irritability of the excitable tissues is exalted by the universal and constant stimulus supplied by the acid to an extent that is just bordering on that which gives rise to rhythmic movement, so that when the violent con- traction is given in response to the mechanical stimulus, the disturbance serves to start the rhythmic movement. ARTIFICIAL RHYTHM. 193 If a paral3^zed nectocalyx, while manifesting its artificial rhythm in acidulated sea-water, be sud- denly transferred to normal sea- water, the move- ments do not cease immediately, but continue for a considerable time. This fact can easily be ex- plained by the very probable, and indeed almost necessary, supposition that it takes some time after the transference to the normal saa-water for the acid to be washed out from contact with the ex- citable tissues. Sooner or later, however, as we should expect, in the normal sea-water the rhythmic movements entirely cease, and the bell becomes quiescent, with a normal irritability as regards single stimuli. If it be now again transferred to the acidulated water, after a short interval the rhythmic movements will again commence, and so on during several repetitions of this experiment, until the irritability of the tissues has finally become destroyed by the influence of the acid. Other chemical irritants which I have tried pro- duce substantially similar effects on the paralyzed bell of Sarsia. I shall, therefore, only wait to describe the influence of one of these irritants, the action of which in some respects diflers from that of acids, and which I have found to be one of the most unfailing in its power to produce the rhythmic movements in question. This irritant is glycerine, and in order to produce its full effect it requires to be added to the sea-water in about the proportion of five per cent. The manifestation of artificial rhythm in solutions of this kind is quite unfailing. It begins after an exposure of from fifteen to thirty 194* JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. seconds, and continues for a variable number of seconds. It generally begins with powerful con- tractions, of a less shivering character than those which are produced by acids, and therefore still more closely resembling the normal swimming motions of the unmutilated animal. Sometimes, however, the first manifestation of the artificial rhythm is in the form of a few gentle rhythmic contractions, to be followed by a few seconds of quiescence, and then by the commencement of the sustained bout of strong contractions. In either case, when the bout of strong contractions sets in, the rate of the rhythm becomes progressively and rapidly increased, until it runs up into incipient tetanus. The rate of the rhythm still quickening, the tetanus rapidly becomes more and more pro- nounced, till at last the bell becomes quiescent in tonic spasm.* If the bell is still left in the glycerine solution nothing further happens; the tissues die in this condition of strong systole. But if the bell be transferred to normal sea-water immediately after, or, still better, slightly before the tonic spasm has become complete, an interesting series of pheno- mena is presented. The spasm persists for a long * Sometimes, however, the order of events is slightly different, the advent of the spasm being more sudden, and followed by a mitigation of its severity, the bell then exhibiting what is more usually the first phase of the series, namely, the occurrence of the locomotor-like contractions. Occasionally, also, rhythmical shivenng contractions may be seen superimposed on the general tonic contraction, either in a part or over the whole of the con- tractile tissues. ARTIFICIAL RHYTHM. 195 time after the transference without undergoing any change, the length of this time depending on the stage in the severity and the spasm at which the transference is made. After this time is passed, the spasm becomes less pronounced than it was at the moment of transference, and a reversion takes place to the rhythmic contractions. The spasm may next pass off entirely, leaving only the rhythmic contractions behind. Eventually these too fade away into quiescence, but it is remarkable that they leave behind them a much more persistent exaltation of irritability than is the case with acid. For in the case of glycerine, the paralyzed bell which has been exposed to the influence of the irritant and afterwards become quiescent in normal sea-water, Avill often continue for hours to respond to single stimuli with a bout of rhj^thmic contrac- tions. This effect of glycerine in producing an extreme condition of exalted irritability is also rendered apparent in another way ; for if, during the soaking stage of the experiment — i.e. before the first of the rhythmic contractions has occurred — the bell be nipped with the forceps, the effect may be that of so precipitating events that the whole of the rhythmic stages are omitted, and the previously quiescent bell enters at once into a state of rigid tonic spasm. This effect is particularly liable to occur after prolonged soaking in weak solutions of glycerine. As in the case of stimulation by acid, so in that of stimulation by glycerine, the artificial rhythm never begins in any strength of solution until the 196 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISn, AND SEA-URCHINSw epithelial surface has become opalescent to a con- siderable degree. In the case of stimulation by glycerine, the be- haviour of the manubrium is more unequivocal than it is in the case of stimulation by acid. I have therefore reserved till now my description of the behaviour of this organ under the influence of constant chemical stimulation. This behaviour is of a very marked though simple character. The manubrium is always the first to respond to the stimulation, its retraction preceding the first movements of the bell by an interval ot several seconds, so that by the time the bell begins its rhythmic response the manubrium is usually re- tracted to its utmost. The initial response of the manubrium is also rhythmic, and the rhythm which it manifests — especially if the glycerine solution be not over-strong — is of the same slow character which has already been described as manifested by this organ when under the influence of mechanical stimulation. The rhythm, however, is decidedly quicker in the former than in the latter case. Lastly, with regard to the marginal ganglia, it is to be observed that in the case of all the chemical irritants I have tried, if unmutilated specimens of Sarsia be immersed in a sea-water solution of the irritant which is of a sufficient strength to evoke artificial rhythm in paralyzed specimens, the spon- taneity of the ganglia is destroyed in a few seconds after the immersion of the animals, i.e. in a shorter time than is required for the first appearance of ARTIFICIAL RHYTHM. 197 artificial rhythm. Consequently, whether the specimens experimented upon be entire or paralyzed by removal of their margins, the phenomena of artificial rhythm under the influence of chemical stimulation are the same. But although the spon- taneity of the ganglia disappears before the artificial rhythm sets in, such is not the case with the reflex activity of the ganglia ; for on nipping a tentacle of the quiescent bell before the artificial rhythm has set in, the bell will give a single normal response to the stimulation. Hence, in historical order, on dropping an unmuti- lated specimen of Sarsia into a solution of glycerine of the strength named, the usual succession of events to be observed is as follows. First, increased activity of the normal swimming motions, to be quickly followed by a rapid and progressive decrease of such activity, till in about fifteen seconds after the immersion total quiescence supervenes. Four or five seconds later the manubrium begins to re- tract by rhythmical twitches, the rate of this rhythm rapidly increasing until it ends in tonic contraction. When the manubrium has just become fully retracted — or very often a little earlier — the bell suddenly begins its forcible and well-pro- nounced rhythmic contractions, which rapidly in- crease in their rate of rhythm until they coalesce into a vigorous and persistent spasm. If the animal be now restored to normal sea -water, spontaneity will return in a feeble manner ; but there is always afterwards a great tendency displayed by the bell to exhibit shivering spasms instead of norma' 14 108 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. swimming movements in response to natural or ganglionic stimulation. And, as already observed, this peculiarity of the excitable tissues is also well marked in the case of the artificial stimulation of degangiionated specimens under otherwise similar conditions. One further experiment may here be mentioned. Having split open the paralyzed bell of Sarsia along the whole of one side from base to apex of the cone, I suspended the now sheet-like mass of tissue by one corner in the air, leaving the rest of the sheet to hang vertically downwards. By means of a rack-work support I now lowered the sheet of tissue, till one portion of it dipped into a beaker filled with a solution of glycerine of appropriate strength. After allowing this portion to soak in the solution of glycerine until it became slightly opalescent, I dropped the entire mutilated bell, or sheet of tissue, into another beaker containing sea-water. If the exposure to the glycerine solution had been of sufficient duration, I invariably found that in the normal sea-water the rhythmic movements were performed by the whole tissue-mass quite as efficiently as was the case in my other experiments, where the whole tissue-mass, and not merely a por- tion, had been submitted to the influence of the irritant. But on now suddenly snipping ofl[' the opalescent portion of the tissue-mass, i.e. the por- tion which had been previously alone submitted to the influence of the irritant, all movement in the remainder of the tissue-mass instantly ceased. This experiment I performed repeatedly, sometimes ex- ARTIFICIAL RHYTHM. 199 posing a large and sometimes a small portion of the tissue to the influence of the irritant. As I in- variably obtained the same result, there can be no doubt that in the case of chemical stimulation the artificial rhythm depends for its manifestation on the presence of a constant stimulus, and is not merely some kind of obscure fluttering motion which, having been started by a stimulus, is after- wards kept up independently of any stimulus. Such being the case, I naturally expected that if I were to supply a constant stimulus of a thermal kind, I should also obtain the phenomena of arti- ficial rhythm. In this, however, my expectations have not been realized. With no species of Medusa have I been able to obtain the slightest indication of artificial rhythm by immersing the paralyzed animals in heated water. I can only explain this fact by supposing that the stimulus which is sup- plied by the heated medium is of too uniform a character over the whole extent of the excitable tissues; it would seem that in order to produce artificial rhythm there must be a differential in- tensity of stimulation in difTerent parts of the responding tissue, for no doubt even the excitatory influence of acidulated water is not of nearly so uniform an intensity over the whole of the tissue- area as is tbat of heated w^ater. In now quitting the subject of artificial rhythm as it is manifested by the paralyzed bell of Sarsia, it is desirable again to observe that sustained artificial rhythm cannot be produced by means of chemical irritation in the case of any one of the 200 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. species of covered-eyed Medusae that I have met with. In order to evoke any response at all, stronger solutions of the irritants require to be em- ployed in the case of the covered than in that of the naked-eyed Medusa3, and when the responses do occur they are not of so suggestive a character as those which I thought it worth while so fully to describe. Nevertheless, even in the covered-eyed Medusae well marked, though comparatively brief, displays of artificial rhythm may often be observed as the result of constant chemical stimulation. Thus, for instance, in the case of Aurelia, if the paralyzed umbrella be immersed in a solution of glycerine (ten to twenty per cent.), a few rhytlimic pulsa- tions of normal rate are usually given ; but shortly after these pulsations occur, the tissue begins to go into a tetanus, which progressively and rapidly becomes more and more pronounced until it ends in violent tonic spasm. So that the history of events really resembles that of Sarsia under similar cir- cumstances, except that the stage of artificial rhythm which inaugurates the spasm is of a character comparatively less pronounced. Thus far, then, I have detailed all the facts which I have been able to collect with reference to the phenomena of artificial rhythm, as produced by different kinds of constant stimulation. It will not be foro'otten that the interest attaching^ to these facts arises from the bearing which they have on the theory of natural rhythm. My belief is that hitherto the theory of rhythm as due to ganglia has attributed far too nuich importance to the ganglionic ARTIFICIAL RHYTHM. 201 as distinguished from the contractile tissues, and I have founded this belief principally on the facts which have now been stated, and which certainly prove at least this much : that after the removal of the centres of spontaneity, the contractile tissues of the Medus?e display a marked and persistent tendency to break into rhythmic action whenever they are supplied w^ith a constant stimulus of feeble intensity. Without waiting again to indicate how this fact tends to suggest that the natural rhythm of the unmutilated organisms is probably in large part due to that alternate process of exhaustion and restoration of excitability on the part of the contractile tissues, whereby alone the phenomena of artificial rhythm can be explained,* I shall go on to describe some further experiments which were designed to test the question whether the influences which affect the character of the natural rhythm likewise, and in the same manner, affect the character of the artificial rhythm. I took the trouble to perform these experiments, because I felt that if they should result in answering this question in the affirmative, they would tend still further to * It is of importance to point out the fact that some of my previously stated experiments appear conclusively to prove that the natural stimulation which is supplied by the marginal ganglia of the Medusae resembles all the modes of artificial stimulation which are competent to produce artificial rhythm in one im- portant particular; the intensity of the stimulation which the marginal ganglia supply is shown by these experiments to be about the same as that which is required to produce artificial rhythm in the case of artificial stimulation. In proof of this point, I may allude particularly to the observations which are detailed on pp. 134-136. 202 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. substantiate the view I am endeavouring to uphold, viz. that the natural rhythm may be a function of the contractile as distinguished from the gang- lionic tissue. Of the modifying causes in question, the first that I tried was temperature. Having already treated of the effects of tem- perature on the natural rhythm, it will now be sufiicient to say that we have seen these effects to be similar to those which temperature exerts on the rhythm of ganglionic tissues in general. Now, I find that temperature exerts precisely the same influence on the artificial rhythm of deganglionated tissue as it does on the natural rhythm of the un- mutilated animal. To economize space, I shall only quote one of my observations in a table which ex- plains itself. I also append tracings of another obser- vation, to render the difference in the rate of the artificial rhythm more apparent to the eye (Fig. 28). ^rature of water Number of contractions (Fabr.). per minute. 25° 24 45° 40 75° 60 During the whole progress of such experiments the faradaic stimulation was, of course, kept of uniform intensity; so that the progressive ac- celeration is undoubtedly due to the increase of temperature alone. With each increment of tem- perature the rate of the artificial rhythm increases suddenly, just as it does in the case of the natural rhythm. Moreover, there seems to be a sort of rough correspondence between the amount of in- 0 J _ 10 - E^ ( t/3 8 204 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. fluence that any given degree of temperature exerts on the rate of the natural and of the artificial rhythm respectively. Further, it will be remembered that in warm water the natural rhythm, besides being quicker, is not so regular as it is in cold water; thus also it is with the artificial rhythm. Again, water below 20° or above 85° suspends the natural rhythm, i.e. stops the contractions; and the artificial rhythm is suspended at about the same degrees. Lastly, just as there are considerable individual variations in the extent to which the natural rhythm is affected by temperature, so the artificial rhythm is in some cases more influenced by this cause than in others, though in all cases it further resembles the natural rhythm in showing some considerable degree of modification under such influence. On the whole, then, it would be impossible to imagine two cases more completely parallel than are these of the effects of temperature on natural and on artificial rhythm respectively; and as it must be considered in the last degree improbable that all these coincidences are accidental, I conclude that the effects of temperature on the natural rhythm of Medusae (and so, in all probability, on the natural rhythm of other ganglio-muscular tissues) are for the most part exerted, not on the ffanHionic, but on the contractile element. In order to test the effects of gases on the artificial rhytlim, I took a severed quadrant of Aurelia, and floated it in sea-water, with its muscular surface just above the level of the water. ARTIFICIAL RHYTHM. 205 Over the tissue I lowered an inverted beaker filled with the gas the effects of which I desired to ascertain, and by progressively forcing the rim of the beaker into the water I could submit the tissue to various pressures of the atmosphere of the gas I was using. By an appropriate arrangement the electrodes passed into the interior of the beaker, and could then be manipulated from the outside, so as to be properly adjusted on the tissue. In this way I was able to observe that different gases exerted a marked influence on the rate of the artificial rhythm. The following table gives the ratios in the case of one experiment : — Rate of artificial rhythm, in air. 36 per minute. In oxygen. 50 per minute. In carbonic acid. 25 per minute. It may here be observed that to produce these results, both carbonic acid and oxygen must be considerably diluted with air, for otherwise they have the effect of instantaneously inhibiting all response, even to the strongest stimulation. When this is the case, however, irritability returns very soon after the tissue is again exposed to air or to ordinary sea-water. But I desire it to be under- stood that the results of my experiments on the influence of oxygen, both on the natural and on the artificial rhythm, have proved singularly equivocal ; so that as far as this gas is concerned further observations are required before the above results can be accepted as certain. I have still one other observation of a very 206 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISII, AND SEA-UKCHINS. interesting character to describe, which is closely connected with the current views respecting gan- glionic action, and may therefore be more con- veniently considered here than in any other part of this treatise. I have already stated that in no case is the manubrium of a Medusa affected as to its movements by removal of the periphery of the swimming-bell ; but in the case of Sarsia a very interestino- chancre occurs in the manubrium soon after the nectocal^^x has been paralyzed by excision of its margin. Unlike the manubriums of most of the other Medusce, this organ, in the case of Sarsia, is very highly retractile. In fresh and lively speci- mens the appendage in question is carried in its retracted state ; but when the animals become less vigorous — from the warmth or impurity of the water in which they are confined, or from any other cause — their manubriums usually become relaxed. The relaxation may show itself in various degrees in different specimens subjected to the same conditions, but in no case is the degree of relaxa- tion so remarkable as that which may be caused by removing the periphery of the nectocalyx. For the purpose of showing this effect, it does not signify in what condition as to vigour, etc., the specimen chosen happens to be in ; for whether the manubrium prior to the operation be contracted or partially relaxed, within half an hour after the operation it is sure to become lengthened to a considerable extent. In order to shovv^ the surprising degree to which this relaxation mr.y proceed, I insert a sketch of a ARTIFICIAL RHYTHM. 207 specimen both before and after the operation. The (^ketches are of life size, and drawn to accurate measurement (Figs. 29 and 30).* Fig. 29. 30. With regard to this remarkable effect on the manubrium of removing the margin of the necto- calyx, it is now to be observed that in it we appear to have very unexceptionable evidence of such a relation subsisting between the ganglia of the nectocalyx and the muscular fibres of the manubrium as elsewhere gives rise to what is known as muscular tonus. This inter])retation * I may here mention that the fact of the manubrium of Sarsia undergoing this extreme elongation after the removal of the mar- ginal ganglia, serves to render the artificial rhythm of the organ under the influence of injury, as previously described, all the more conspicuous. 208 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. of the facts cannot, I think, be disputed ; and it fully explains why, in the unmutilated animal, the degree of elongation on the part of the manubrium usually exhibits an inverse proportion to the degree of locomotor activity displayed by the bell. I may here state that I have also observed indications of muscular tonus in some of the other Medusae, but for the sake of brevity I shall now restrict myself to the consideration of this one case. To my mind, then, it is an interesting fact that orano^lionic tissue, where it can first be shown to occur in the animal kingdom, has for one of its functions the maintenance of muscular tonus ; but it is not on this account that I now wish to draw prominent attention to the fact before us. Physio- logists are almost unanimous in regarding muscular tonus as a kind of gentle tetanus due to a persistent ganglionic stimulation, and against this opinion it seems impossible to urge any valid objection. But, in accordance with the accepted theory of gan- glionic action, physiologists further suppose that the only reason why some muscles are thrown into a state of tonus by ganglionic stimulation, while other muscles are thrown into a state of rhythmic action by the same means, is because the resistance to the passage of the stimulation from the ganglion to the muscle is less in the former than in the latter case. Here, be it remembered, we are in the domain of pure speculation : there is no experi- mental evidence to show that such a state ot differential resistance as the theory requires actu- ally obtains. Hence we are quite at liberty to ARTIFICIAL RHYTHM. 209 suppose any other kind of difference to obtain, either to the exclusion of this one or in company with it. Such a supposition I now wish to suggest, and it is this — that all rhythmical action being regarded as due (at any rate in large part) to the alternate exhaustion and restoration of excitability on the part of contractile tissues, the reason why continuous ganglionic stimulation produces incipient tetanus in the case of some muscles and rhythmic action in the case of others, is either wholly or partly because the irritability of the muscles in relation to the intensity of the stimulation is greater in the former than in the latter case. If this sup- position as to differential irritability be granted, my experiments on paralyzed Aurelia prove that tetanus would result in the one case and rhythmic action in the other. For it will be remembered that in these experiments, if the continuous faradaic stimulation were of somewhat more than minimal intensity, tetanus was the result; while if such stimulation were but of minimal intensity, the result was rhythmic action. Now, that in the particular case of Sarsia the irritability of the toni- cally contracting manubrium is higher than that of the rhythmically contracting bell is a matter, not of supposition, but of observable fact ; for not only is the manubrium more irritable than the bell in response to direct stimulation of its own sub- stance, but it is generally more so even when the stimuli are applied anywhere over the excitable tissues of the bell. And from this it is evident that the phenomena of muscular tonus, as they 210 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. occur in Sarsia, tend more in favour of the exhaus- tion than of the resistance theor}^* I will now sum up this rather lengthy discussion. The two theories of ganglionic action may be stated antithetically thus : in both theories the accumulation of energy by ganglia is supposed to be a continuous process; but while the resistance theory supposes the rhythm to be exclusively due to an intermittent and periodic discharge of this accumulated energy on the part of the ganglionic tissues, the exhaustion theory supposes that the rhythm is largely due to a periodic process of exhaustion and recovery on the part of the respond- * The evidence, however, is not aUoorefher exclusive of the resistance theory, for it is quite possible that in addition to the hi^h irritability of the manubrium there may be conductile lines of low resistance connecting this organ with the marginal ganglia. I entertain this supposition because, as explained in my Eoyal Society papers, I see reason to believe that the natural swimming movements of Sai'sia are probably in part due to an intermittent discharge of the ganglia. I think, therefore, that in this par- ticular case the ganglia supply a tolerably constant stimulation to the manubrium, while it is only at intervals that their energy overflows into the bell, and that the higher degree of irritability on the part of the manubrium ensures the tonic response of this organ at a small cost of nervous energy. How far the rhythm of the nectocalyx is to be attributed to the resistance mechanism of the ganglia, and how far to the alternate exhaustion and recovery of the contractile tissues, I think it is impossible to determine, seeing that it is impossible exactly to imitate the natural gan- glionic stimulation by artificial means. But it is, I think, of importance to have ascertained at least this much, that in Sarsia the tonus of one organ and the rhythm of another, which appa- rently both received their stimulation from the same ganglia, must at any rate in part be attributed to a differential irrita- bility of these organs, as distinguished from their dilierent ial stimulation. ARTIFICIAL RHYTHM. 211 ing tissues. Now, I submit that my experiments have proved the former of these two theories inade- quate to explain all the phenomena of rhythm as it occurs in the Medusae ; for these experiments have shown that even after the removal of the only ganglia which serve as centres of natural stimulation, the excitable tissues still continue to manifest a very perfect rhythm under the influence of any mode of artificial stimulation (except heat), which is of a constant character and of an inten- sity sufficiently low not to produce tetanus. And as 1 have proved that the rhythm thus artificially produced is almost certainly due to the alternate process of exhaustion and recovery which I have explained, there can scarcely be any doubt that in the natural rhythm this process plays an important part, particularly as we find that temperature and gases exert the same influences on the one rhythm as they do on the other. Again, as an additional reason for recognizing the part which the con- tractile tissues probably play in the production of rhythm, I have pointed to the fact that in the great majority of cases in which rhythmic action occurs the presence of ganglia cannot be suspected. For it is among the lower forms of life, where ganglia are cei-tainly absent, and where the functions of stimulation and contraction appear to be blended and diffused, that rhythmic action is of the most frequent occurrence; and it is obvious with how much greater difficulty the resistance theory is here beset than is the one I now propose. Granted a dififused power of stimulation with a diffused power 212 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. of response, and I see no essential difference between the rhythmic motions of the simplest organism and those of a deganglionated Medusa in acidulated water. Lastly, the facts relating to the tonus of the manubrium in Sarsia furnish very striking, and I think almost conclusive, proof of the theory which I have advanced. CHAPTER IX. POISONS. 1. Chloroform. — My observations with regard to the distribution of nerves in Sarsia led me to in- vestio-ate the order in which these connections aie destroyed, or temporarily impaired, by amiesthetics. The results, I think, are worth recording. In Sarsia the following phases always mark the progress of antesthesia by chloroform, etc. — 1. Spon- taneity ceases. 2. On now nipping a tentacle, pulling the manubrium, or irritating the bell, a single locomotor contraction is given in answer to every stimulation. (In the unan?esthesiated animal a series of such contractions would be the result of such stimulation.) 3. After locomotor contractions can no longer be elicited by stimuli, nipping a tentacle or the margin of the bell has the effect of causing the manubrium to contract. 4. After stimu- lation of any part of the nectocalyx (including tentacles) foils to pi-oduce response in any part of the or^fanism, the manubrium will continue its re- sponse to stiumli ap[)lied directly to itself. 2. Nitrite of Aniyl. — On Sarsia the effect of this 15 214 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. agent is much the same as that of chloroform — the description just given being quite as applicable to the effects of tlie nitrite as to those of chloroform. Before the loss of spontaneity supervenes, the rate of the rh^^thm is increased, while the strength of the pulsations is diminished. Tiaropsis diademata, from the fact of its present- ing a very regular rhythm and being but of small size, is a particularly suitable species upon which to conduct many experiments relating to the effect of poisons. On this species the nitrite in appro- priate (i.e. in very small) doses first causes irregularity and enfeeblement of the contractions, together with quickening of the rhythm. After a short time, a gradual cessation of the swimming motions be- comes apparent — these motions dying out more gradually, for example, than they do under the influence of chloroform. Eventually each pulsation is marked only by a slight contraction of the muscular tissue in the immediate neighbourhood of the margin. If the dose has been stronger, however, well-marked spasmodic contractions come on and obliterate such gradual working of the poison. In all cases irritability of all parts of the animal persists for a long time after entire cessation of spontaneous movements — perhaps for three or four minutes in not over-poisoned animals ; but eventu- ally it too disappears. On being now transferred to noi'inal sea- water, the process of recovery is slower than it is after ansesthesiation by chloroform. It is interesting, moreover, to observe, that just as the power of co-ordination was the first thing to be POISONS. 215 affected by the nitrite, so it is the last thing to return during recovery. 3. Caff tin. — The eilects of caffein on Sarsia may be best studied by immersing the animals in a saturated sea-water solution of the substance. In such solutions the Medusi^ float to the surface, in consequence of their lower specific gravity. I therefore used shallow vessels, in order that the margins of the nectocalyces might rest in the level of the water that was thoroughly saturated. The immediate effect of suddenly immersing Sarsia in such a solution is very greatly to increase the rate of the pulsations, and, at the same time, to diminish their potency. The appearance presented by the swimmino- motions is therefore that of a flutterinof nature ; and such motions are not nearly so effectual for progression as are the normal pulsations in unpois;oric tubercle (Fig 32, 771). Continuing our examination of the upper surface, we may observe, when we use a lens, a number of small pincer-like organs scattered about between the calcareous nodules, or attached to the spines ; these are known as the pedicellariae. Each Fig. 32. Upper surface of a Star-fish {Astropeckn). Hist.") (From Cassell's " Nat. consists of a stalk serving to support a pair of forceps or pincers, and the whole being provided with muscles, the stalk is able to sway about and the pincers to open and shut (Fig. 33). The entire mechanism is therefore clearly adapted to seizing and holding on to something ; but what it is that these curious orcjans ai-e thus adapted to seize, and 256 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. therefore of what use they are in the economy of the animal, has long been a standing puzzle to naturalists. I hope presently to be able to show- that we have succeeded in doing something towards the solution of this puzzle. Fig. 33.— redicellariae (magnified). (From Cassell's «• Nat. Hist."; Turninir now to the under surface of our Star-fish (Fig. 34), %^e observe that the mouth is situated in the centre of the disc, and that from this mouth as a centre there radiate five grooves or furrows, Avhich severally extend to the tips of each of the five rays. On each side of these grooves there are numerous actively moving membraneous tubes, which may be protruded or retracted by being filled or em[)tied w^ith fluid. These are used for crawding, and I shall therefore call them the feet, or pedicels. So much, then, for the external surface of a Star- fish. If, now, we examine the internal stincture, we STAR-FISH AND SEA-UKCHINS. 257 find that the central mouth leads by a short oeso- plia lior ul Echinus shell. (From Cassell's " Nat. Hist.") a hard shell, which, if we break, we find to be hollow^ and filled with fluid (Fig. 40\ The fluid 2G6 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. closely resembles sea-water, but is, nevertheless, richly corpusculated ; it coagulates when exposed to the air, and otherwise shows that it is something more than mere sea- water. If we look closely into the shell which has been deprived of its spines, we find that it is composed of a great number of small hexagonal j)lates (Fig. 41), the edges of which fit £0 closely together that the whole shell is converted into a box, Avhich, when the animal is alive, is water-tight, as we have proved by submitting the contained fluid to hydrostatic pressure, under which »-j3LV > n .1 / O 0 J r" mQl Fig. 41.— A portion of the external shell of an Echinus denuned of ppines and slightly magnified, showing the arrangement of the plates, the balls in the ball- and-socket joints of the spines, and the holes through which the ambulacral feet are protruded. (From Cas.-ell's "Nat. Hist.") circumstances there is no leakage until the pressui-e is sufficient to burst the shell Nevertheless, if we look closely at the dned shell of an Echinus, we shall .see that it is not an absolutely closed box ; for we shall see that the hexagonal plates are so arranged as to give rise to five double rows of holes or pores (Fig. 41), which extend symmetrically from pole to pole of the animal (Fig. 39). It is through these holes that the tube-feet are protruded ; so that if we imagine a pentagonal species of Star-fish to be curved into the shape of a hollow spheroid, and then converted into a calcareous box with holes STAR-FISH AND SEA-UKCHINS. 2C7 left for its feet to come through, we should have a mental picture of an Echinus. It would only be necessary to add the curious apparatus of teeth (Figs. 40 and 42), which occurs in the Echinus, to increase the size of tlie spines and pedicellariae, and to make a few other such minor alterations ; but in all its main features an Echinus is merely a Star- fish with its five rays calcified and soldered together so as to constitute a riicid box. This echinoid type itself varies considerably among its numerous constituent species as to size, shape, length and thickness of the spines, etc. ; but Fig. -12.— Teeth of Kcliiiiu^- (Irom CisseU's " Nat. Hist.") I need not wait to go into these details. Again, merely inviting momentary attention to the develop- mental history of these animals, I may remark that the phases of development through which an indi- vidual Echinoderm passes are not less varied and remarkable than are the permanent forms eventually assumed by the sundry species. Katura I Movem en fs. Turning now to the physiology of the Star-fish group, I shall begin by describing the natural movements of the animals. 26S JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. Takinor the common Star-fish as our stavtingr- point, I have ahxady explained the mechanism of its ambulacral system. The animals usually crawl in a determinate direction, and when in the course of their advance the terminal feet of the advancing ray — which are used, not as suckers, but as feelers, protruded forwards — happen to come into contact with a solid bod}^ the Star-fish may either continue its direction of advance unchanged, or may turn towards the body which it has touched. Thus, for instance, while crawling along the floor ot a tank, if the terminal feet at the end of a ray happen to touch a perpendicular side of the tank, the animal may either at once proceed to ascend this perpendicular side, or it may continue its progress along the floor, feeling the perpendicular side with the end of its rays perhaps the whole way round the tank, and yet not choosing, as it were, to ascend. In the cases where it does ascend and reaches the surface of the water, a Star-fish very often performs a number of peculiar movements, which we may call acrobatic (Fig. 43). On reaching the surface, the animal does not wish to leave its native element — in fact, cannot do so, because its sucking feet can only act under water — and neither does it wish again to descend into the levels from which it has just ascendcJ. It, therefore, begins to feel about for rocks or sea-weeds at the surface, by crawlinof alon^' the side of the tank, and every now and then throwing back its uppermost ray or rays along the surface of the water to feel for any solid support that may be within reach. If it finds one, STAR-FISH AND SEA-URCHINS. 269 it may very likely attach its uppermost rays to it, and then, letting go its other attachments, swing from the one support to the other. The activity and co-ordination manifested in these acrobatic movements are surprising, and give to the animal an almost intelligent appearance. In Astropecten the ambulacral feet have become partly rudnnentary, inasmuch as they have lost Fig. 43.— Natural movements of a Star-fieh on reaching the surface of water. their terminal suckers (Fig. 44). These Star-fish, therefore, assist themselves in locomotion by the muscular movements of their rpys, while they use their suckeiiess feet to run along the ground some- what after the manner of centipedes. It is to be noticed, however, that altliough the feet have lost their suckers, the Star-hsh is still able to make them adhere io solid surfaces in a comparatively inefficient 270 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. manner, by constricting the tube on one side after it has brought this side into opposition with the solid surface (Fig. 45). In the Brittle-stars the ambulacra! feet have been still more reduced to rudiments, and are of no use at all, cither as suckers or for assisting in locomotion. These Star-fish have, therefore, adopted another method of locomotion, and one which is a great Fig. 44. — .\ pedicel of Astropecten (magnified), showing the absence of any terminal sucker. Fig. 45.— The same, showing the method of extemporizing a sucker. improvement upon the slow crawling of other members of the Star-fish group. The rays of the Brittle-stars are very long, flexible, and muscular, and by their combined action the animal is able to shuffle along fiat horizontal surfaces. When it desires to move rapidly, it uses two of its opposite arms upon the horizontal floor with a motion like 46) ; at each stroke the animal swimming (Fi STAR-FISH AND SEA-UPtCIIIXS. 271 adv^anccs with a leap or bound about the distance of two inches, and as the strokes follow one another rapidly, the Star-lish is able to travel at the rate of six feet per minute. A common Star-lish, on the other hand, with its slow crawling method of Fig. 46.— Natunil movements of a Brittle-star when proceeding along a solid horizontal surface. progression, can only go two inches per minute. Some of the Comatuhe, in which the muscularity of the rays has proceeded still further, are able actually to swim in the water by the co-ordinated movements of their rays.* * In this cas9 the locomotion of a Star-fish comes to be per- formed on the same plan or method as that of a Jelly-fish — the five rays performing, by their co-ordinated action, the same function as a swimming-bell. It is a cnrionsly interesting fact, that although no two plans or mechanisms of locomotion could well bo imagined as more fundamentally distinct than those which are respectively characteristic of these two groups of 272 JELLY-FISH, STARFISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. The Ecliiniis crawls in the same way as the common Star-fish; but besides its long suckers it also uses its spines, wdiich by their co-ordinated action push tlie animal along. The suckers, more- over, in being protruded from all sides of a globe instead of from the under side of a flat oi'ganism, are of much more use as feelers than they are in the Star-fish. Therefore, wdiile advancing, the feet facing the direction of advance are always kept extended to their fullest length, in order to feel for any object which the animal may possibly be approaching. When a perpendicular surface is reached, the Echinus may either ascend it or not, as in the case of the Star-fish. While walking, the animal keeps pretty persistently in one direction of advance. If it be partly rotated by the hand, it does not continue in the same direction, but continues its own movements as before ; so that, for instance, if it is turned half round, it will proceed in a direction opposite to that in which it had previously been going. When at rest, some of the feet are used as anchors, and others protruded as feelers. Regarded from the standpoint of the evolutionist, we have here an interesting series of gradations. At one end of the series we have the Echinus with its rays all united in a box-like rigid shell. At the other end of the series we have the Brittle-stars and Comatuhie with their highly muscular and animals, nevertheless in this particular case and in virtue of special modilication, a Star-fish should have adopted the plan or mechanism of a Jelly-fish. STAR-FISH AND SEA-UllCIIINS. 2/3 mobile rays. J\[icl\vay in tlie series we have Astro- pecten and the common Star-fish, where the rays are flexible and mobile, though not nearly so much so as in tlie Brittle-stars. Now, the point to observe is, that in correlation with this graduated difference in the mobility of the rays, there is a correspond- ingly graduated difference in the development of the ambulacral system of suckers. For in Echinus this system is seen in its most elaborate and efficient form ; in the common Star-fish the suckers are still the most important organs of locomotion, though the muscularity of the rays has begun to tell upon the development of the specially ambulacral system, the suckers not being so long or so powerful as they are in Echinus. Lastly, the Brittle-stars and ComatuhB have altogether discarded the use of their sucking feet in favour of the much more efficient organs of locomotion supplied by their muscular rays ; and, as a consequence, their feet have dwindled into useless rudiments, while the rays have become limb-like in their activity. There is only one other point in connection with the natural movements of the Echinodermata which it is necessary for me to touch upon. All the species when turned upon their backs are able again to right themselves ; but seeing, as I have just observed, that tlie or!:»'ans of locomotion in the different species are not the same, the methods to which these species have to resort in executing the righting manoeuvre are correspondingly diveise. Thus, the Brittle-stars can easily perfoJ'm the need- ful manoeuvre by wriggling some of their snake- 274 .IFXLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. like arms under the inverted disc, and heaving the whole bod}^ over by the mere muscularit}^ of these organs. The common Star-fish, however, experiences more difficult}', and executes the manoeuvre mainly by means of its suckers. That is to say, it twists round the tip of one or more of its rays (Fig. 47) until the ambulacral feet there situated are able to Fig. 47. — Natural righting rmvements of ronimon Star-fish. get a firm hold of the floor of the tank (a) ; then, by a successive and similar action of the ambulacral feet further back in the series, the whole ray is twisted round (b), so that the ambulacral surface cf the end is applied flat against the floor of the tank (c). The manoeuvre continuing, the semi-turn STAR-FISH AND SEA-URCHINS. 275 or spiral travels ])rogressing all the way down the ra}'. Usually two or three adjacent rays perform this mantieuvre simultaneously ; but if, as some- times ha])pens, two opposite rays should begin to do so, one of them soon ceases to continue the manoeuvre, and one or both of the ra3^s adjacent to the other takes it up instead, so assisting and not thwarting the action. The spirals of the co- operating rays being invariably turned in the same direction (Fig. 47, a, b, and c), the result is, when they have proceeded sufficiently far down the rays, to drag over the remaining rays, which then abandon their hold of the bottom of the tank, so as not to offer any resistance to the lifting action oi the active rays. The whole movement does not occupy moie than half a minute. As a general rule, the rays are from the first co-ordinated to effect the rIo*htin2f movement in the direction in which it is finally to take place — the ra3^s which are to be the active ones alone twistinof over, and so twisting that all their spirals turn in the same direction. A Star-fish (Astropecten) which is intermediate between the Brittle-star and the common Star-fish, in that its ambulacral feet are partly aborted (having lost their suckers, as shown in Fig. 44) and its rays more mobile than those of the common Star- fish, rio'hts itself after the manner shown in Fi^;. 48, where the animal is represented as standing on the tips of four of its rays, while the fifth one is just about to bo thrown upwards and over the others, in order to cai ry with it the two adjacent rays, and so 276 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHmS. eventually to overbalance the system round the fulcrum supplied by the tips of the other two rays, and thus bring the animal down upon its ventral surface. But it is in the case of Echinus that these right- ing movements become most interesting, from the fact that they are so much more difficult to accom- Fig 48. — Righting movements of Astr.'pecten. plish than they are in the case of the Star-fishes. For while a Star-tish is provided with flat, flexible, and muscular rays, comprising a small and light mass in relation to the motive power, an Echinus is a rigid, non-muscular, and globular mass, whose only motive power available for conducting the mancEuvre is that which is supplied by its re- STAR-FISH AND SEA-URCHIXS. 277 latively feeble ambulacral feet. It is, therefore, scarcely surprisino- that unless the specimens chosen for these observations are perfectly fresh and vigorous, they are unable to riglit themselves at all ; they remain permanently inverted till they die. But if the specimens are fresh and vigorous, they are sooner or later sure to succeed in right- ing themselves, and their method of doing so is always the same. Two, or perhaps three, adjacent rows of suckers are chosen out of the five, as the rows which are to accomplish the task (Fig. 49). As iilll|lNII|lllil||![iiIplll|l||l|l|||||||iiill!ll|ilil!il|||l|i|illllllllllll|ll|i|lll|!|l|llll|l|||K^^^ Fig. 49. many feet upon the rows as can reach the floor of the tank are protruded downwards and fastened firmly to the floor; their combined action then serves to tilt the globe slightly over in their own direction, the anchoring feet on the other or opposite rows meanwhile releasing their hold of the tank to admit of this tilting (Fig. 50). The effect of this tilting is to enable the next feet in the active ambulacral rows to touch the floor of the tank, and, when they have established their hold, 19 2/8 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. tliev assist in increasing^ the tilt ; then the next feet in the series lay hold, and so on, till the globe Fig. 50. slowly but steadily rises upon its equator (Fig. 51). The difficulty of raising such a heavy mass into Fig. 51. this position by means of the slender motive power STAR-FISH AND SEAUrtCHINS. 279 available can be at once appreciated on witnessing the performance, so that one is surprised, notwith- standing the co-ordination displayed by all the suckers, that they are able to accomplish the work assigned to them. That the process is in truth a very laborious one is manifest, not only from the extreme slowness with which it takes place, but also because, as already observed, in the case of not perfectly strong specimens complete failure may attend the efforts to reach the position of resting on the equator — the Echinus, after rearing up a certain height, becoming exhausted and again falling back upon its ab-oral pole. I\roreover, in some cases it is interesting to observe that when the equator position has been reached with difficulty, the Echinus, as it were, gives itself a breathing space before beginning the movement of descent — drawing in all its pedicels save those which hold it securely in the position to which it has attained, and remaining in a state of absolute quiescence for a prolonged time. It then suddenly begins to protrude all its feet again, and to continue its manoeuvre. At any time during such a period of rest, a stimulus of any kind will immediately determine a recom- mencement of the manoeuvre. It will be perceived that as soon as the position just described has been attained, gravity, which had hitherto been acting in opposition to the righting movement, now begins to favour that movement. It might, therefore, be anticipated that the Echinus would now simply let go all its attachments and allow itself to roll over into its natural position 280 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. But an Echinus will never let go its attachments without some uro^ent reason, seemingr to be above all things afraid of being rolled about at the mercy of currents ; and therefore in this case it lets itself down almost as slowly as it raised itself up. So gently, indeed, is the downward movement effected, that an observer can scarcely tell the precise moment at which the ricrhtins: is concluded. There- fore, in the downward movement, the feet, which at the earlier part of the manoeuvre were employed lililllilillllllllllllli!llyiiniiii!,i'!'!!"i!!ill!!'l!'iiO Fig. 52. successfully in rearing the globe upon its equator, are now employed successfully in preventing its too rapid descent (Fig. 52). Several interesting questions arise with reference to these righting movements of Echinus. First of all we are inclined to ask what it is that determines the choice of the rows of feet which are delegated to effect the movements. As the animal has a geometrical form of perfect symmetry, we might suppose that when it is placed upon its pole, all the STAR-FISH AND SEA-URCHIXS. 281 five rows of feet would act in antag-onism to one another; for there seems nothinix more to determine either the action or the inaction of one row rather than another. Indeed, if there were any moral philosophers among the Echinoderms, they might point with triumph to the fact of their being able to right themselves as an irrefutable argument in favour of the freedom of the Echinoderm will. " We are in form," they might say, " perfectly geometrical, and our feet-rows are all arranged with perfect symmetry ; therefore there is no reason, apart from the sovereign freedom of our choice, why we should ever use one set of feet rather than another in exe- cuting this important movement." And indeed, I do not see how these Echinoderm philosophers could be answered by any of the human philosophers, who, with less mathematical data and with less physio- logical reason, employ analogous arguments to prove the freedom of the human will. Physiologists, however, would give these Echinoderm philosophers the same answer that they are in the habit of giving to the human philosophers, viz. that although the physiological conditions are very nicely balanced, they are never so nicely balanced as to leave positively nothing to determine which rows of feet — that is to say, which sets of nerves — shall be used. And in this connection I may observe that on making a number of trials it becomes apparent in the case of certain individual specimens that they manifested a marked tendency to rotate always in the same direction, or to use the same set of foot-rows for the purpose of righting themselves. 282 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-UUCIIINS. In tliese individual specimens, therefore, we must conclude tliat the foot-rows thus employed are selected because of some slight accidental prepotency or superiority over the others ; the animal has, as it wore, thus much individual character as the result of a slight prepotency of some of its nerve-centres over the others. Another question of still more interest arises out of these righting movements, namely, that as to their prompting cause. This question, however, I shall defer till later on, since it cannot be answered without the aid of experiments as distinguished from observation. Stmiulation. In now quitting our observations on the natural movements of the Echiiiodermata, and beo-innino^ an account of the various experiments which we have tried upon these animals, I shall first take the experiments in stimulation. All the Echinodermata seek to escape from injury. Thus, for instance, if a Star-fish or an Echinus is advancing continuously in one direc- tion, and if it be pricked or otherwise irritated on any part of an excitable surface facing the direction of advance, the animal immediately reverses that direction. There is one point of special interest concerning these movements of response to stimulation. The form of the animals and the distribution of the nervous system being, as I have before said, of geometrical regularity, it follows that by applying two stimuli simultaneously STAR-FISH AND SEA-URCHINS. 283 on two (lifrerent aspects of the animal, the combined result uf these two stimuli is that of furnishing a very pretty instance in physiology of the physical principle of the parallelogram of forces. Thus, for instance, if two stimuli of equal intensity be applied simultaneously at the opposite sides of a globular Echinus, the animal begins to walk in a direction at right angles to an imaginary line joining these two points. And, generally, wherever the two points of simultaneous stimulation may be situated, the direction of the animal's advance is the diagonal between them. As showino- in more detail how very delicate is the physiological balancing of stimuli which may be produced in these organisms, and consequently the manner in which we are able to play, as it were, upon their geometrically disposed nervous systems in illustration of the mechanical principle of the composition of forces, I shall quote a series of observations. " 1. Scraped with a scalpel the equator of an Echinus at two points opposite to each other — animal crawled at right angles to the line of injury. "2. Similarly scraped at the ab-oral pole — no effect. There was no reason why injury here should determine escape in one direction rather than in another. " 3. Scraped similarly near the oral pole, and half-way between pole and equator — little or no effect. "4. Scraped in rapid succession five equatorial and equidistant injuries — Echinus crawled actively in one determinate direction; the equal and equi- 284 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. distant injuries all round the globe neutralized one another. ** 5. Scraped a band of uniform width all the way round the equator — same result as in 4. "6. Band of injury in same specimen was then widened in the side facing' the direction of crawlino^ — no effect. Still further widened — slii^ht chans^e of direction, and, after a time, persistent cravvding away from the widest part of the injured zone. Repeated this experiment on other specimens by scraping round the whole equator, and simultane- ously making one part of the zone of injury wider than the rest — same result; the animal crawled away from the greatest aQnount of injury. " 7. Scraped on one side of the equator, and, after the animal had been cravv^ling in a direct line from the source of irritation for a few minutes, similarly scraped equator on the opposite side — animal re- versed its direction of crawling ; it crawled away from the stimulus supplied latest. "8. Scraped a number of places on all aspects of the animal indiscriminately — direction of ad- vance uncertain and discontinuous, with a strons: tendency to rotation upon vertical axis." These observations show conclusively that the whole external surface, not only of the soft and fleshy Star-fish, but even of the hard and rigid Echinus, is everj^where sensitive to stimulation. Closer observation shows that this sensitiveness, besides being so general, is highly delicate. For if any part of the external surface of an Echinus is lightly touched with the point of a needle, all the feet, STAR-FISH AND SEA-URCHINS. 285 spines, and pedlcellari?e within reach of that part, and even beyond it, immediately converge and close in u[)on the needle, grasp it, and hold it fast. This simul- taneous movement of sucli a little forest of prehensile organs is a very beautiful spectacle to witness. In executing it the pedicellarici3 are the most active, the spines somewhat slower, and the feet very much slower. The area affected is usually about half a square inch, although the pedicellarise even far beyond this area may bend over towards the seat of stimulation, which, however, from their small size they are not able to reach. And here we have proof of the function of the pedicellariie — proof which w^e consider to be im- portant, because, as I have before said, the use of these organs has so long been a puzzle to naturalists. In climbing perpendicular or inclined surfaces of rock, covered with waving sea-weeds, it must be of no small advantage to an Echinus to be provided on all sides with a multitude of forceps, all mounted on movable stalks, which instantaneously bring their grasping forceps to bear upon and to seize a passing frond. The frond being thus arrested, the spines come to the assistance of the pedicell arise, and both together hold the Echinus to the support furnished by the sea-weed. Moreover the sea-weed is thus held steady till the ambulacral feet have time also to establish their hold upon it w^th their sucking discs. That the grasping and arresting of fronds of sea- weed in this way for the purposes of xocomotion constitute an important function of the pedicellarige, may at once be rendered evident 286 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. experimentally b}^ drawing a piece of sea-weed over the surface of a healthy Echinus in the water. The moment the sea-weed touches the surface of the animal, it is seen and felt to be seized by a number of these little grasping organs, and — unless torn away by a greater force than is likely to occur in currents below the surface of the sea — it is held steady till the ambulacral suckers have time to establish their attachments upon it. Thus there is no doubt that the pedicellarise are able efficiently to perform the function which we regard as their chief function. We so regard this function, not merely because it is the one that we observe these organs chiefly to perform, but also because we find that their whole physiology is adapted to its performance. Thus their multitudinous number and ubiquitous situation all over the external surface of the animal is suggestive of their being adapted to catch something which may come upon them from any side, and which may have strings and edges so fine as to admit of being enclosed by the forceps. Again, the instantaneous activity with which they all close round and seize a moving body of a size that admits of their seizing it, is suggestive of the objects which they are adapted to seize being objects which rapidly brush over the surface of the shell, and therefore objects which, if they are to be seized at all, must be ionized instantaneously. Lastly, we find, on experiment- ing upon pedicellaria^, whether in sitit or when separated from the Echinus, that the clasping action of the forccp.^ is precisely adapted to the function STAR-FISH AND SEA-URCHINS. 287 which we are considering ; for not only is the force exerted by the forceps during their contraction of an astonishing amount for the size of the organ (the serrated mandibles of the trident pedicelJaiia? holding on with a tenacity that can only have reference to some objects liable to be dragged away from their grasp), but it is very suggestive that this wonderfully tenacious hold is spontaneously relaxed after a minute or two. This is to say, the pedi- cellaripe tightly fix the object which they have caught for a time sufficient to enable the ambulacral suckers to establish their connections with it, and then they spontaneously leave go ; their grasp is not only so exceedingly powerful while it lasts, but it is as a rule timed to suit the requirements of the pedicels.* Concerning the physiology of the pedicellariie little further remains to be said. It may be stated, however, that the mandibles, which are constantly * A further proof that this is at least one of the functions of the pedicellariae is furnished by a simple experiment. If an Echinus is allowed to attach its feet to a glass plate held just above its ab-oral pole, and this plate be then raised in the water so that the Echinus is freelj suspended in the water by means of its feet alone, the animal feels, as it were, that its anchorage is insecure, and actively moves about its unattached feet to seek for other solid surfaces. Under such circumstance? it may be observed that the pedicellariae also become active, and especially so near the surface of attachment, as if seeking for pieces of sea-weed. If a piece is presented to them, they lay hold upon it with vigour. Of course the pedicellariae may also have other functions to perform, and ia a Star-fish Mr. Sladen has seen them engaged in cleaning the surface of the animal ,• but we cannot doubt that at least in Echinus their main function is that which we have stated. 283 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. swaying about upon their contractile stalks as if in search for something to catch, will snap at an object only if it touches the inner surface of one or more of the expanded mandibles. Moreover, in the larger pediccllariiie, a certain part of the inner surface of the mandibles is much more sensitive to contact than is the rest of that surface; this part is a little pad about one-third of the way down the mandible : a delicate touch w^ith a hair upon this part of any of the three mandibles is certain to determine an immediate closure of all the three. It is obvious that there is an advantage in the sen- sitive area, or zone, being placed thus low enough down in the length of the mandibles to ensui-e that the whole apparatus will not close upon an object till the latter is far enough within the grasp of the mechanism to give this mechanism the best possible hold. If, for instance, the tips of the mandibles were the most sensitive parts, or even if their whole inner surfaces were uniformly sensitive, the ap- paratus would be constantly closing upon objects when these merely brushed past their tips, and therefore closing prematurely for the purpose of grasping. But, as it is, the appaiatus is admirably adapted to waiting for the best possible chance of getting a secure hold, and then snapping upon the object witli all the quickness and tenacity of a spring- trap. Another point w^orth mentioning is that if, after closure, any one or more of the mandibles be gently stroked on its outer surface near the base, all the mandibles are by this stimulation usually, though STAR-FISH AND SEA-UnCIIfNS. 289 not invariably, induced again to expand. This is the only pait of the whole organ the stimulation of which thus exerts an inhibitory influence on the contractile mechanism. If there is any functional purpose served by such relaxing influence of stimu- lating this particular part of the apparatus, we think it can only be as follows. When a portion of sea-weed brushes this particular part, it must be well below the tips of the mandibles, and therefore in a position where it, or some over-lying portion, may soon pass between the mandibles, if the latter are open; hence when touched in this place the mandibles, if closed, open to receive the sea-weed, should any part of it come within their cavity. Turning next to experiments in stimulation with reference to the spines, I may observe that we have found these organs to be, physiologically considered, highly remarkable and interesting, from the fact that they display co-ordinated action in a degree which entitles them to be regarded as a vast multi- tude of limbs. Thus, for instance, if an Echinus be taken out of the water and placed upon a table, it is no longer able to use its feet for the purpose of locomotion, as their suckers are only adapted to be used under water. Yet the animal is able to pro- gress slowly by means of the co-ordinated action of its spines, which are used to prop and push the globe-like shell along in some continuous direction. If, while the animal is thus slowly progressing, a liojhted match be held near it, facinor the direction of advance, as soon as the animal comes close enough to feel tho heat, all the spines begin to make the 290 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. animal move away in the opposite direction. More- over, as showing the high degree in which the action of the spines is co-orJihated, I may mention that there is an urchin-like form of Echinoderra, which is called Spatangus, and wdiich differs fi om the Echinus in having shorter feet and longer spines. When, therefore, a Spatangus is inverted, it is unable to right itself by means of its feet, as these are too short to admit of being used for this purpose ; but, never- theless, the animal is able to right itself by means of the co-ordinated action of its long spines, these being used successively and laboriously to prop and push the animal over in some one definite direction. The process takes a very Jong time to accomplish, and there are generally numerous failures, but the creature perseveres until it eventu- ally succeeds. Coming now to stimulation with reference to the feet, we find that when a drop of acid, or other severe stimulation, is applied to any part of a row of protruded pedicels, the entire row is immediately retracted, the pedicels retracting successively from the seat of irritation— so that if the latter be in the middle point of the series, two series of retractions are started, proceeding in opposite directions simul- taneously ; the rate at which they travel is rather slow. This process of retraction, however, although so complete within the ray irritated, does not extend to the other rays. But if the stimulus be applied to the centre of the disc, upon the oral surface of the animal, all the feet in all the rays are more or less retracted— the process of retraction radiating stah-fish and sea-urchins. 291 serially fi-om the centre of stimulation. The influ- ence of the stimulus, however, diminishes perceptibly with the distance from the centre. Thus, if weak acid be used as the irritant, it is only the feet near the bases of the rays that are retracted ; and even if very strong acid be so used, it is only the feet as far as one-half or two-thirds of the way up the rays that are fully retracted — the remainder only having their activity impaired, wdiile those near the tip may not be adected at all. If the drop of acid be placed on the dorsal, instead of the ventral surface of the disc, the effect on the feet is found to be just the converse ; that is, the stimulus here applied greatly increases the activity of the feet. Further experi- ments show that this effect is produced by a stimu- lus applied anywhere over the dorsal aspect of the animal ; so that, for instance, if a drop of acid be placed on the skin at the edge of a ray, and there- fore just external to the row of ambulacral feet, the latter will be stimulated into increased activity ; whereas, if the drop of acid had been placed a very small distance past the edge of the ray, so as to touch some of the feet themselves, then the whole row would have been drawn in. We have here rather an interesting case of antagonism, which is particularly well marked in Astropecten, on account of the active writhing movements which the feet exhibit when stimulated by an irritant placed on the dorsal surface of the animal. It may be added that in this antagonism the inhibitory function is the stronger ; for when the feet are in active motion, owin^i' to an irritant actino- on the dorsal surface. 292 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AXD SEA-URCHINS. they may be reduced to immediate quiescence — i.e. retracted — by placing another irritant on the ven- tral surface of the disc. Similarly, if retraction has been produced by placing the irritant on the ventral surface of the disc, activity cannot be again induced by placing another drop of the irritant on the dorsal surface. Now, if we regard all these facts of stimulation taken together, it becomes evident that the external organs of an Echinoderm — feet, spines, and pedicel- lari?e — are all highly co-ordinated in their action ; and therefore the probability arises that they are all held in communication with one another by means of an external nervous plexus. Accordingly we set to work on the external surface of the Echinus to see whether we could obtain any evi- dence of such a plexus microscopically. This we succeeded in doing, and afterwards found that Pro- fessor Loven had already brietly mentioned such a plexus as having been observed by him. The plexus consists of cells and fibres, closely distri- buted all over the surface of the shell, immediately under the epidermal layer of cells (Figs. 53, 54, 5o), and it sends fibres all the way up the feet, spines, and pedicellarise. As it seemed to us important to investigate the physiological properties of this plexus. Professor Ewart and I made a number of further experiments, an account of which will now lead us on to the next division of our subject, or that of section. STAR-FISH AND SEA-URCHINS. 293 rig. 53. Exiciiiul neivj-plexusof Edifiius. Fig. 54. Structure of a nerve-trunk of Eclnnus. r^>-~.^ Fig. 55. Nerve-cells lying among the muscnlir fibres at the base of a spine in Echiuus. 20 " 294« JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. Sidlon, 1. Slar-iish. — Single rays cletacliecl from the organism crawl as fast and in as determinate a direction as do the entire animals. They also crawl up perpendicular surfaces, and sometimes away from injuries ; but the}^ do not invariably, or even generally, seek to escape from the latter, as is so certain to be the case with entire animals. Lastly, when inverted, separated rays right them- selves as quickly as do the unmutilated organisms. Dividing the nerve in any part of its length has the effect, whether or not the ray is detached from the animal, of completely destroying all physio- logical continuity between the pedicels on either side of the line of division. Thus, for instance, if the nerve be cut across half-way up its length, the row of pedicels is at once physiologically bisected, one-half of the row becoming as independent of the other half as it would were the whole ray divided into two parts : that is to say, the distal half of the row may crawl while the proximal half is retracted, or vice versa; and if a drop of acid be placed on either half, the serial contraction of the pedicels in that half stops abruptly at the line of nerve- division. As a result of tliis complete physiological severance, when a detached ray so mutilated is inverted, it experiences much greater difficulty in righting itself than it does before the nerve is divided. The line of nerve-injury lies flat upon the floor of the tank, Avhile the central and distal portions of the ray, i.e. the portions on either side STAR-FISH AND SEA-URCHINS. 295 of that line, assume various movements and shapes. The central portion is particularly apt to take on the form of an arch, in which the central end of the severed ray and the line of nerve-section constitute the points of support (tetanus ?) (Fig. oQ), or the central end may from the first show paralysis, from which it never recovers. The distal end, on the other hand, usually continues active, twisting about in various directions, and eventually fasten- ing its tip upon the floor of the tank to begin the spiral movement of righting itself This movement Fi g. 56. Movements performed by a detached ray of a Star-fish, in which the central nerve-trunk is divided. then continues as far as the line of nerve-injury, where it invariably stops (Fig. oQ). The central portion may then be dragged over into the normal position, or may remain permanently inverted, according to the strength of pull exerted by the distal portion ; as a rule, it does not itself assist in the righting movement, although its feet usually continue protruded and mobile. Thus, the effect of a transverse section of the nerve in a ray is that of completely destroying physiological continuity between the pedicels on either side of the section. 296 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. The only other experiments in nerve-section to which the simple anatomy of a Star- fish exposes itself is that of dividing the nerve-ring in the disc; or, which is virtually the same thing, while leaving this intact, dividing all the nerves where the}^ pass from it into the rays. In specimens mutilated by severino: the nerves at the base of each of the five rays, or by dividing the nerve ring between all the rays, the animal loses all power of co-ordination among its rays. When a common Star-fish is so mutilated it does not crawl in the same determinate manner as an unmutilated animal, but, if it moves at all, it moves slowly and in various directions. When inverted, the power of effecting the righting manoeuvre is seen to be gravely impaired, although eventually success is ahvays achieved. There is a marked tendency, as compared with unmutilated specimens, to a promiscuous distribution of spirals and doublings, so that instead of a definite plan of the manoeuvre beino^ formed from the first, as is usually the case with unmutilated specimens, such a plan is never formed at all ; among the five rays there is a continual change of un-coordinated move- ments, so that the righting seems to be eventually effected by a mere accidental prepotency of some of the righting movements over others. Appended is a sketch of such un-coordinated movement, taken from a specimen which for more than an hour had been twisting its rays in various directions (Fig. 57). Another sketch is appended to show a form of bending which specimens mutilated as described are very apt to manifest, especially just after the STAR-FISH AND SEA-URCHINS. 297 operation. When placed upon their dorsal surface, they turn up ail their rays with a peculiar and exactly similar curve in each, which gives to the animal a somewhat tulip-lilvc form (Fig. 58). This form is never assumed by unmutilated specimens, and in mutilated ones, although it may last for a long time, it is never permanent. In detached rays Fig. 5/ Un-coordinated movcm':>nts of a vStar-fish, in which the nerves of all the rays have been divided. this peculiar curve is also frequently exhibited ; but if the nerve of such a ray is divided at any point in its length, the curve is restricted to the distal portion of the ray, and it stops abruptly at the line of nerve-section. When entire Star- fish are mutilated by a section of each nerve-trunk half-way up each ray, and the animal is then placed upon its back, the tetanic contractiju of the muscles in the rays 298 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. before mentioned as occurring under this form of section in detached rays, has the effect, when now occurring in all the rays, of elevating the disc from the floor of the tank. This opisthotonous-like spasm is not, however, permanent ; and the distal ends of the rays forming adhesions to the floor of the tank, the animal eventually rights itself, though Fig. 58. Form freqnentl}- assumed by Soar-fi^h under similar ciramistances. much more slowly than unmutilated specimens. After it has righted itself, although it twists about the distal portions of the rays, it does not begin to craAvl for a long time, and when it does so, it crawls in a slow and indeterminate manner. Star-fish so mutilated, however, can ascend perpendicular surfaces. STAR-FISII AND SEA-URCHINS. 299 Tl]c loss of co-ordination between the rays caused by division of the nerve-ring in the disc is rendered most conspicuous in Brittle-stars, from the circum- stance that in locomotion and in righting so much here depends upon co-ordinated muscular contrac- tion of the rays. Thus, for instance, when a Brittle- star has its nerve-ring severed between each ray, an interesting series of events follows. First, there is a long period of. profound shock — spontaneity, and even irritability, being almost suspended, and the rays appearing to be rigid, as if in tetanic spasm. After a time, feeble spontaneity returns — ■ the animal, however, not moving in any determinate direction. Irritability also returns, but only for the rays immediately irritated, stimulation of one ray causing active writhing movements in that ray, but not affecting, or only feebly affecting, the other rays. The animal, therefore, is quite unable to escape from the source of irritation, the aimless movements of the ra3's now forming a very marked contrast to the instantaneous and vigorous leaping movements of escape which are manifested by unmutilated specimens. Moreover, un mutilated specimens will vigorously leap away, not only from stimulation of the rays, but also from that of the disc ; but those with their nerve-ring cut make no attempts to escape, even from the most violent stimulation of the disc. In other words, the disc is entirely severed from all physiological connection witli the rays. If the nerve-ring be divided at two points, one on either side of a ray, that ray becomes physio- 300 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-UKCHINS. logically separated from the rest of the organism. If the two nerve-divisions are so placed as to in- clude two ailjacent rays — i.e. if one cut is on one side of a ray and the other on the further side of an adjacent ray — then these two rays remain in physiological continuity with one another, although they suffer physiological separation from the other three. When a Brittle-star is completely divided into two portions, one portion having two arms and the other three, both portions begin actively to turn over on their backs, again upon their faces, again upon their backs, and so on alternately for an in- definite number of times. These movements arise from the rays, under the influence of stimulation caused by the section, seeking to perform their natural movements of leaping, which however end, on account of the weight of the other rays being absent, in turninor themselves over. An entire Brittle-star when placed on its back after division of its nerve-rino^ is not able to rig^ht itself, owinor to the destruction of co-ordination among its rays. Astropecten, under similar circumstances, at first bends its rays about in various ways, with a pre- ponderant disposition to the tulip form, and keeps its ambulacral feet in active movement. But after half an hour, or an hour, the feet generally become retracted and the rays nearly motionless — the animal, like a Brittle-star, remaining permanently on its back. In this, as in other species, the eflect of dividing the nerve-ring on either side of a ray is that of destroying its physiological connection with the rest of the animal, the feet in that ray, STAR-FISH AND SEA-URCHINS. 301 although still remaining feebly active, no longer taking part in any co-ordinated movement — that ray, therefore, being merely dragged along by the others. Under this division it only remains further to be said, that section of the nerve-ring in the disc, or the nerve-trunks of the rays, although, as we have seen, so completely destroying physiological con- tinuity in the rows of ambulacral feet and muscular system of the animal, does not destroy physiological continuity in the external nerve-plexus ; for how- ever much the nerve-ring and nerve- trunks may be injured, stimulation of the dorsal surface of the animal throws all the ambulacral feet and all the muscular system of the rays into active movement. This fact proves that the ambulacra! feet and the muscles are all held in nervous connection with one another by the external plexus, without reference to the integrity of the main nerve-trunks. 2. Echini. — Section of external surface of shell. — If a cork-borer be applied to the external surface of the shell of an Echinus, and rotated there till the calcareous substance of the shell is reached, and therefore a continuous circular section of the over-lying tissues effected, it is invariably found that the spines and pedicellarias within the circular area are physiologically separated from the con- tiguous spines and pedicellarige, as regards local reflex excitability. That is to say, if any part of this circular area be stimulated, all the spines and pedicellarice within that area immediately respond to the stimulation in the ordinary way; while none 302 JELLY-FISII, STAR-FISH, AN^D SEA-URCHINS. of the spines or pcdicellariaB surrounding the area are affected. Similarly, if any part of the shell external to the circumscribed area be stimulated, the spines and pedicellarije within that area are not affected. These facts prove that the function which is manifested by those appendages of localizing and gathering round a seat of stimulation, is exclu- sively dependent upon the external nerve-plexus. It is needless to add that in this experiment it does not signify of what size or shape or by what means the physiological island is made, so long as the destruction of the nervous plexus by a closed curve of injury is rendered complete. In order to ascer- tain whether, in the case of an unclosed curve of injury, any irradiation of a stimulus would take place round the ends of the curve, we made sundry kinds of section. It is, however, needless to describe these, for they all showed that, after injury of a part of the plexus, there is no irradiation of the stimulus round the ends of the injury. Thus, for instance, if a short straight line of injury be made, by drawing the point of a scalpel over the shell, say along the equator of the animal, and if a stimulus be afterwards applied on either side of that line, even quite close to one of its ends, no effect will be exerted on the spines or pedicellarijB on the other side of the line. This complete inability of a stimulus to escape round the ends of an injury, forms a marked contrast to the almost unlimited degree in which such escape takes place in the more primitive nervous plexus of the Medusse. Although the nervous connections on which the STAR- FISH AND SEA-URCHINS. 303 spines and pcdiccllarire depend for their function of localizino- and closino- round a seat of stimulation are thus shown to be completely destroyed by injury of the external plexus, other nervous connec- tions, upon which another function of the spines depends, are not in the smallest degree impaired by Buch injury. The other function to which I allude is that which brings about the general co-ordinated action of all the spines for the purposes of locomo- tion. That this function is not impaired by injury of the external plexus is proved by the fact that if the area within a closed line of injury on the surface of the shell be strongly irritated, all the spines over the whole surface begin to manifest their peculiar bristling movements, and by this co- ordinated action rapidly move the animal in a straight line of escape from the source of irritation ; the injury to the external plexus, although com- pletely separating the spines enclosed by it from their neighbouring spines as regards what may be called their local function of seizinn^ the instrument of stimulation, nevertheless leaves them in undis- turbed connection with all the other spines in the organism as regards what may be called their universal function of locomotion. Evidently, therefore, this more universal function must depend upon some other set of nervous con- nections; and experiment shows that these are dis- tributed over all the internal surface of the shell. Our mode of experimenting was to divide the animal into two hemispheres, remove all the internal oigan« of both hemispheres (these operations pro- 304 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. ducing no impairment of any of the functions of the pedicels, spines, or peflicellari?e), and then to paint with strong acid the inside of the shell — com- pletely washing out the acid after about a quarter of a minute's exposure. The results of a number of experiments conducted on this method may be thus epitomized : — The effect of painting the back or inside of the shell with strong acid {e.g. pure HCl) is that of at first strongly stimulating the spines into bristling- movements, and soon afterwards reducing them to a state of quiescence, in which they lie more or less tiat, and in a peculiarly confused manner that closely resembles the appearance of corn wdien " laid " by the wind. The spines have now entirely lost both their spontaneity and their power of responding to a stimulus applied on the external surface of the shell — i.e. their local reflex excita- bility, or powder of closing in upon a source of irri- tation. These effects may be produced over the whole external surface of the shell, by painting the whole of the internal surface ; but if any part of the internal surface be left unpainted, the cor- i-esponding part of the external surface remains uninjured. Conversely, if all the internal surface be left unpainted except in certain lines or patches, it will only be corresponding lines and patches on the external surface that suffer injury. It makes no difference wdiether these lines or patches be painted in the course of the ambulacral feet, or anywhere in the inter-ambulacral spaces. The above remarks, which have reference to the STAR-FISH AND SEA-URCHINS. 305 spines, apply equally to the pedlcellarire, except that their s[)ontaneity and reiiex irritability are not destroyed, but only impaired. Some hours alter the operation it usually happens that the spontaneity and reiiex irritability of the spines return, though in a feeble degree, and also those of the pedicellari^B, in a more marked degree. This applies especially to the reflex irritability of the pedicellarise ; for while their spontaneity does not return in full degree, their reflex irritability docs — or almost in full degree. These experiments, therefore, seem to point to the conclusions — 1st, that the o'eneral co-ordination of the spines is dependent on the integrity of an internal nerve-plexus ; 2nd, tiiat the internal plexus is everywhere in intimate connection with the ex- ternal; and ord, that complete destruction of the former, while profoundly influencing the functions of the latter, nevertheless does not wholly destroy them. Professor Ewart therefore undertook carefully to examine the internal surface of the shell, to see whether any evidence of this internal nervous plexus could be found microscopically, and, after a great deal of trouble, he has succeeded in doing so. But as he has not yet published his results, I shall not forestall them further than to say that this internal plexus spreads all over the inside of the shell, and is everywhere in communication with the external plexus by means of fibres which pass be- tween tlie sides of the hexagonal plates of which the shell of the animal is composed. Thus we can o;)6 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FIGH, AND SEA-UKCHINS. understand ho'vV it is that when a portion of the external plexus is isolated from the rest of that plexus as a residt of the cork-borer experiment, the island still remains in communication with the nerve-centres which preside over the co-ordination of the spines, as proved by the fact of the Echinus using its spines to escape from irritation applied to the area included within the circle of injury to the external plexus produced b}^ the cork-borer. Now, where are these nerve-centres situated ? We have just seen that we have evidence of the presence of such centres somewhere in an Echinus, seeing that all the spines exhibit such perfect co-ordination in their movements. Where, then, are these centres ? Seeinor that in a Star-fish the ravs are co-ordinated in their action by means of the pentagonal ring in the disc, analogy pointed to the nervous ring round the mouth of an Echinus as the part of the nervous system which most probably presides over the co-ordinated action of the spines. Accordingly, we tried the effect of removing this nervous ring, and immediately obtained conclusive proof that this was the centre of which we were in search ; for as soon as the nervous ring was removed, the Echinus lost, completely and permanently, all power of co- ordination among its spines. That is to say, after this operation these organs were never again used by the animal for the purposes of locomotion, and no matter how severe an injury we applied, the Echinus, when placed on a table, did not seek to escape. But the spines were not wholly paralyzed, STAR-FISH AND SEA-URCTIINS. 307 or motionless. On the contrary, their power of spontaneous movement continued unimpaired, as did also their power of closing round a seat of irritation on the external surface of the sliell. The same remark applies to the pedicellarine, and the explanation is simple. It is the external nervous plexus which holds all the spines and pedicellariee in comnumication with one another as by a network; so that when any part of this network is irritated, all the spines and pedicellari?e in the neighbourhood move over to the seat of irritation. On the other hand, it is the internal plexus which serves to unite all the spines to the nerve-centre which surrounds the mouth, and which alone is competent to co- ordinate the action of all the spines for the purposes of locomotion. It remains to consider whether the ambulacral feet exhibit any general co-c^linated action, and, if so, whether this likewise depends upon the same nerve-centre. The fact already mentioned, that during pro- gression an Echinus uses some of its feet for crawl- ing and others for feeling its way, is enough to suggest that all the feet are co-ordinated by a nerve-centre. But in order to be quite sure about the fact of there being a general co-ordination among all the feet, we tried the following experiments. I have already described the rii-htino,- movements which are performed by an Echinus when the animal is inverted, and it will be remembered that in this animal the manoeuvre is effected by means of the fet;t alone. At first sicht this midit almost 308 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. seem suMlcient to prove the fact of a general co- ordination among the feet ; but further reflection will show that it is not so. For the feet being all arranged in rec>u]ar series, when one row begins to effect the rotation of the globe, it may very well be that its further rotation in the same direction is due only to the fact that the slight tilt produced by the jmlling of the tirst feet in the series A, B, C gives the next feet in the series D, E, F an opportunity of reaching the floor of the tank; their adhesions being established, they would tend by their pulling to increase still furtlier the tilt of the globe, thus giving the next feet in the series an oppoi'tunity of fastening to the floor of the tank, and so on. In order, therefore, to see whether these righting movements were due to nervous co-ordination among the feet, or merely to the accident of the serial arrangement of the feet, we tried the experi- ments which I shall now detail. First of all we took an Echinus, and by means of a thread suspended it upside-down in a tank of water half-way up the side of the tank, and in such a way that only the feet on one side of the ab-oral pole were able to reach the perpendicular wall of the tank. These feet as quickly as possible estab- lished their adhesions to the pei-pendicular wall, and, the thread being then removed, the Echinus was left sticking to the side of the tank in an in- verted position by means of the ab-oral ends of two adjacent feet-rows (Fig. 50). Under these circum- stances, as we should expect from the previous ex- periments, the animal sets about righting itself as STAR- FISH AND SEA-URCIIINS. 309 quicAly as possible. Now, if the righting action of the Icct were entirely and only of a serial character, the rigliiing would require to be performed by rear- ing the animal upwards; the etfect of foot after foot in the same rows being applied in succession to the side of the tank, would require to be that of rotating: the lobular shell ao-ainst the side of the tank towards the surface of the Avater, and therefore against the action of gravity. This is sometimes clone, Avhich proves that the energy required to per- form the feat is not more than a healthy P^chinus can expend. But much more frequently the Echinus adopts another device, and the only one by which it is possible for him to attain his pui'pose without the labour of rotating upwards: he rotates laterally and downwards in the form of a spiral. Thus, let us call the five feet-rows, 1, 2, 3, 4, and 5 (Figs. 59, 60, 61), and suppose that 1 and 2 are in use near their ab-oral ends in holding the animal inverted against the perpendicular side of a tank. The downward spiral rotation would then be eflected by gradually releasing the outer feet in row 1, and simultaneously attaching the outer feet in row 2 (i.e. those nearest to row 3, and furthest from row 1), as far as possible to the outer side of that row. The eflcc' of this is to make the globe roll far enough to that side to enable the inner feet of row^ o {i.e. those nearest to row 2), when fully protruded, to touch the side of the tank. They establish their adhesions, and the residue of feet in row 1, now leaving go their hold, these new adhesions serve to roll the globe still further round in the same 21 310 JELLY-FISH, STAR- FISH, AND SEA-UKCHINS. STAR-FISII AND SEA-URCHINS. 311 direction of lateral rotation, and so the process pro- ceeds from row to row ; but the globe does not merely roll along in a horizontal direction, or at the same level in the water, for each new row that comes into action takes care, so to speak, that the Fig 61. Figs. 59, 60, and 61 are righting movements of Echinns on a perpendicular suiface. feet which it employs shall be tliose which are as far below the level of the feet in the row last em- ployed as their length wdien fully protruded (i.e. their power of touching the tank) renders possible. The rotation of the orlobe thus becomes a double 312 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. one, lateral and downwards, till the animal assumes its normal position with its oral pole against the perpendicular tank wall. So considerable is the rotation in the downward direction, that the normal position is generally attained before one complete lateral, or equatorial, rotation is completed. The result of this experiment, therefore, implies that the righting movements are due to something more than the merely successive action of the seiies of feet to which the work of righting the animal may happen to be given. The same conclusion is pointed to by the results of the following experi- ment. A number of vigorous Echini were thoroughly shaved with a scalpel over the whole half of one hemispliere, i.e. the half from the equator to the oral pole. They Avere then inverted on their ab-oral poles. The object of the experiment Avas to see what the Echini which were thus deprived of the lower half of three feet-rows would do when, in executing their righting manoeuvres, they attained to the equatorial position and then found no feet wherewith to continue the manoeuvre. The result of this experiment was first of all to show us that the Echini invariably chose the unmutilated feet- rows wherewith to right themselves. Probably this is to be explained, either by the general principle to which the escape from injury is due — viz. that injury inflicted on one side of an Echinoderm stimulates into increased activity the locomotor organs of the opposite side, — or by the consideration that destruction of the lower half of a row very STAR- FISH AND SEA-UllCHINS. 313 probaMy ineluces some degree of shock in the remaining half, and so leaves the corresponding parts of the unmutilated rows prepotent over the mutilated one. Be tliis as it may, however, we found that the difficulty was easily overcome by tilting the animal over upon its mutilated feet-rows sufficiently far to prevent the unmutilated rows from reaching the floor of the tank. When held steadily in this position for a short time, the muti- lated rows established their adhesions, and the Echinus was then left to itself Under these cir- cumstances an Echinus will always continue the manoeuvre along the mutilated feet-rows with which it was begun, till the globe reaches the posi- tion of resting upon its equator, and therefore arrives at the line where the shaved area com- mences. The animal then remains for hours in this position, with a gradual but continuous motion backwards, which appears to be due to the suc- cessive slipping of the spines — these organs in the righting movements being always used as props for the ambulacral feet to pull against while rearing the globe to its equatorial position, and in perform- ing this function on a slate floor the spines are liable often to slip. The only other motion ex- hibited by Echini thus situated is that of a slow rolling movement, now to one side and now to another, according to the prepotency of the pull exerted by this or that row of ambulacral feet. Things continue in this way until the slow back- ward movement happens to bring the animal against some side of the tank, when the uninjured 3 14 JELLY-FISH, STAR-IISII, AND SEA-URCHINS. rows of ainbulacral feet immediately adhere to the surface and rotate the animal upwards or horizon- tally, until it attains the normal position. But if care be taken to prevent contact with any side of the tank, the mutilated Echinus will remain propped on its equator for days ; it never adopts the simple expedient of reversing the action of its mutilated feet- rows, so as to bring the globe again upon its ab-oral pole and get its unmutilated feet- rows into action. From this we may conclude that the righting movements of the pedicels are due, not to the merely serial action of the pedicels, but to their co-ordina- tion by a nerve-centre acting under a stimulus so pplied by a sense of gravity ; for if the move- ments of the pedicels were merely of a serial character, we should not expect that the equatorial position, having been attained under these circum- stances, should be permanently maintained. We should not expect this, because after a while the pedicels, which are engaged in maintaining the globe in its equatorial position, must become ex- hausted and relax their hold, when those next behind in the series would lay hold of the bottom of the tank, and so on, the rotation of the globe thus proceeding in the opposite direction to that in which it had previously taken place. On the other hand, if the righting movements of the pedi- cels are due to co-ordination proceeding from a nerve-centre acting under a sense of gravity, we should expect the animal under the circumstajices mentioned to remain permanently reared upon its STAR-FISH AND SEA-URCHINS. 315 eqTiator; for this would show that the nerve-centre was always persistently, though fruitlessly, endea- vouring to co-ordinate the action of the absent feet. Further, as proof that the ambulacral feet of Echinus are under the control of some centralizinof apparatus when executing the righting manoeuvre, we may state one other fact. When the righting maoenuvre is nearly completed by the rows engaged in executing it, the lower feet in the other rows become strongly protruded and curved downwards, in anticipation of shortly coming into contact with the floor of the tank when the riohtinoj manoeuvre shall have been completed (see Fig. 52, p. 280). This fact tends to show that all the ambulacral feet of the animal are, like all the spines, held in mutual communication with one another by some central- izing mechanism. But the best proof of all that the feet in executing the righting manoeuvre are under the influence of a co-ordinating centre, is one that arose from an experiment suggested to me by Mr. Francis Darwin, and which I shall now describe. Mr. Darwin having kindlj^ sent the apparatus which his father and himself had used in their experiments on the geotropism of plants, it was employed thus. A healthy Echinus was placed in a large bottle filled to the brim with sea- water, and having been inverted on the bottom of the bottle, it was allowed in that position to establish its adhesions. The bottle was then corked and mounted on an upright wheel of the apparatus whereby, by means of clock- work, it could be kept in continual slow rotation 016 JELLY-FISH; STAR-FJSrr, AND SEA-URCHINS. in a vertical plane. The object of this was to ascertain whether the continuous rotation in a vertical plane would prevent the animal from right- ing itseU' (because confusing the nerve-centres which, under ordinary circumstances, could feci by their sense of gravity wliich was up and which was down), or would still allow the animal to right itself (because not interfering with the serial action of the feet). AVell, it was found that this rotation of the whole animal in a vertical plane entirely ])re vented the righting movements during any length of time that it might be continued, and that tiiese movements were immediately resumed as soon as the rotation was allowed to cease. This, moreover, was the case, no matter what phase of the righting manoeuvre the Echinus might have reached at the moment when the I'otation began. Thus, for instance, if the globe were allowed to liave reached the position of resting on its equator befoi-e the rotation was commenced, the Echinus would remain motionless, holding on with its equa- torial feet, so long as the rotation was kept up. Therefore, there can be no question that the am- bulacral feet are all under the influence of a co- O'dinating nerve-centre, quite as much as are the spines. But, on the other hand, experiments show that the centre in this case is not of so localized a character as it is in the case of the spines ; for when the nerve-ring is cut out, the co-ordina- tion of the feet, although impaired, is not Avholly destroyed. Take, for instance, the case of the righting manoiuvre. The effect of cutting out the STAR- FISH AND SEA-URCHINS. 817 nerve-ling is that of entiicly destroying the ability to peiforni this manoRiivre in the case of the majority of specimens ; nevertheless about one in ten continue able to perform it. Again, if an Echinus is divided into two hemispheres by an incision carried from pole to pole through any meridian, the two hemispheres wiU live for days, crawling about in the same manner as entire animals; if their ocular plates are not injured, they seek the light, and when inverted they right themselves. The same observa- tions apply to smaller segments, and even to single detaclied rows of ambulacral feet. The latter are, of course, analogous to the single detached rays of a Star-fish, so far as the S3^stem of ambulacral feet is concerned ; but, looking to the more complicated apparatus of locomotion (spines and pedicellariye), as well as to the rigid consistence and awkward shape of the segment — standing erect, instead of lying flat — the appearance presented by such a seg- ment in locomotion is much more curious, if not surprising, than that presented by the analogous part of a Star-fish under similar circumstances. It is still more surprising that such a fifth-part seg- ment of an Echinus will, when proj'jped up on its ab-oral pole (Fig. 62), right itself (Fig. 03) after the manner of larger segments or entire animals. They, however, experience more difficulty in doing so, and very often, or indeed generally, fail to coujplete the manoeuvre. On the whole, then, we may conclude that the nervous system of an Echinus consists (1) of an external plexus which serves to unite all the feet, 818 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. ,iiii„Hiiiuiiiiiiiiiiiiiiiiiiiiiii||MMmiuiiiiiiiiiiiiiilllllllillllilllllllllliiiHW Figs. 62 and 63.-Higbting and ambulacral movements of severed segments of Ecliinuo. STAR-FISH AND SEA-Ur.CIIINS. 319 spines, and pedicellarire together, so that they all ai)proxiinate a point of irritation sifAiated anywhere in that plexus ; (2) of an internal nervous plexus which is everywlicre in communication through the thickness of the shell with the external, and the function of which is that of bringing the feet, spines, and probably also the pedicellarise into relation with the o'reat co-ordinatino^ nerve-centre situated O CD round the mouth ; (3) of central nervous matter which is mainly gathered round the mouth, and there presides exclusively over the co-ordinated action of the spines, and in large part also over the co-ordinated action of the feet, but which is further in part distributed along the courses of the main nerve-trunks, and so secures co-ordination of ieet even in separated segments of the animal. Special Senses. Before concluding, I must say a few words on the experiments whereby we sought to test for the presence in Echinoderms of the special senses of sight and smell. We have found unequivocal evidence of the Star- fish (with the exception of the Brittle-stars) and the Echini manifesting a strong disposition to crawl tow^ards, and remain in, the light. Thus, if a large tank be completely darkened, except at one end where a narrow slit of light is admitted, and if a number of Star-fish and Echini be scattered over the floor of the tank, in a few hours the whole number, with the exception of perhaps a few per 320 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. coiit, will be found congfue^'.ated in the narrow slit of light. The source we used was diffused day- light, which was admitted through two sheets of glass, so tliat the thciTiial rays might be considered practically excluded. The intensit>j of the light which the Echinoderms are able to perceive may be very feeble indeed ; for in our first experiments we boarded up the face of the tank with ordinary pine- wood, in order to exclude the light over all parts of the tank except at one narrow slit between two of the boards. On takino- down the boards we found, indeed, the majority of tlie specimens in or near the slit of light; but we also found a number of other specimens gatliering all the way along the glass face of the tank that was immediately behind the pine-boards. On repeating the experiment with blackened boards, this was never found to be the case ; so there can be no doubt that in the first experiments the animals were attracted by the faint glimmer of the white boards, as illuminated by the veiy small amount of light scattered from the narrow slit through a tank, all the other sides of which were black slate. Indeed, towards the end of tlie tank, where some of the specimens were found, so feeble must have been the intensity of this glimmer, that we doubt whether even human eyes could have discerned it very distinctly. Owing to the prisms at our command not having sufficient dispersive power for the experiments, and not wish- ing to rely on the uncertain method of employing coloured glass, we were unable to ascertain how the Echinoderms might be affected by different rays. STAR- FISH AND SEA-URCHINS. 321 On removing with a pointed scalpel the eye-spots from a nunber of Star-fish and Echini, without othervvise injuring t1ie animals, the latter no longer crawled towards the liifht, even thouo^h this were admitted to the tink in abundance; but they crawled promiscuously in all directions. On the other hand, if only one oat of the five eye-spots were left intact, the animals crawled towards the light as before. It may be added that single detached rays of Star-fish and fifth-part segments of Echini crawl towards the light in the same manner as entire animals, provided, of course, that the eye-spot is not injured. The presence of a sense of smell in Star-fish was proved by keeping some of these animals for several days in a tank without food, and then presenting them with small pieces of shell-fish. The Star-fish immediately perceived the proximity of food, as shown by their immediately crawling towards it. Moreover, if a small piece of the food were held in a pair of forceps and gently withdrawn as the Star- fish approached it, the Minimal could be led about the floor of the tank in any direction, just as a hungry dog could be led about by continually withdrawing from his nose a piece of meat as he continually follows it up. This experiment, how- ever, was only successful with Star-fish which had been kept fasting for several days ; freshly caught Star-fish were not nearly so keen in their manifesta- tions, and indeed in many cases did not notice the food at all. Desiring to ascertain whether the sense of smell 322 JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. were localized in any particular organs, as we had found to be the case with the sense of sight, I first tried the effect of removing the five ocelli. This produced no difference in the result of the above experiment witli hungry Star-fish, and therefore I next tried the effect of cutting oft* the tips of the rays. The Star-fish behaving as before, I then pro- gressively truncated the rays, and thus eventually found that the olfactory sense was etjually dis- tributed throughout their length. The question, however, still remained whether it was equally dis- tributed over both the upper and the lower sufifaces. I therefore tried the eff"ect of varnishing the upper surface. The Star-fish continued to find its food as before, wdiich showed that the sense of smell was distributed along the lower surface. I could not try the converse experiment of varnishing this surface, because I should thereby have hindered the action of the ambulacral feet. But by another method I was able nearly as well to show that the upper surface does not participate in smelling. This method consisted in placing a piece of shell-fish upon the upper surface and allowing it to rest there. When this was done, the Star-fish made no attempt to remove the morsel of food by brushing it off" with the tips of its rays, as is the habit of the animal when any irritating substance is ^^pj^lied to this surface. Therefore I conclude that the upper or dorsal surface of a Star-fish takes no part in minis- tering to the sense of smell, which by the experi- inent of varnishing this sui-face, and also by that of progressively truncating the rays, is proved to STAR-FISH AND SEA-URCHINS. 323 be distributed over the whole of the ventral or lower surface of the animal. For I must add that severed rays behave in all these respects like the entire organisms, although they are disconnected from the mouth and disc. As this chapter has already extended to so great a length, I omit from it any account of some further experiments which I tried concerning the effects of nerve-poisons upon the Echinodermata. A full record of these experiments may be found in the publications of the Linnean Society. FINIS. D. APPLETON & CO/S PUBLICATIONS. GEORGE J. ROMANES'S WORKS. MENTAL EVOLUTION IN MAN: Origin of Human Faculty. One vol., 8vo. Cloth, $3.00. This work, which follows "Mental Evolution in Animals," by the same au- thor, considers the probable mode of genesis of the human mind from the mind of lower animals, and attempts to show that there is no distinction of kind be- tween man and brute, but, on the contrary, that such distinctions as do exist all admit of bein? explained, with respect to their evolution, by adequate psycho- logical analysis. "The vast array of facts, and the sober and solid method of argument em- ployed by Mr. Romanes, will prove, we think, a great gilt to knowledge."— Saturday Bevietv. JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. Being a Research on Primitive Nervous Systems. 12rao. Cloth, $1.75. " Although I have throughout kept in view the requirements of a general reader, I have also sought to render the book of service to the working physi- ologist, by brinsring together in one consecutive account all the more important observations and results which have been yielded by this research."— ^ic^rac^ from Pre] ace. "A profound research into the laws of primitive nervous systems conducted by one of the al)lest Enr. C. K. Adams's Manual of Historical Literature. ANTS, BEES, AND WASPS. A Record of Observations on the Habits of the Social Hymenoptera. With Colored Plates. l£mo. Cloth, 12.00. "This volume contains the record of various experiments made with ants, bees, and wasps during the last ten years, with a view to test their mental con- dition and powers of sense. The author has carefully watched and marked par- ticular insects, and has had their nests under observation for long periods— (ne of his ants' nests havinsr been under constant inspection ever since 1874. His ob-^ervations are made principally upon ants, because they show more power and flexibility of mind ; and the value of his studies is that they belong to the de- partment of original research." ON THE SENSES, INSTINCTS, AND INTELLIGENCE OF ANIMALS, WITH SPECIAL REFERENCE TO INSECTS. " International Scientific Series." With over One Hundred Illustra- tions. 12mo. Cloth, $1.75. The author has here collected some of his recent observations on the senses and intelligence of animals, and especially of insects, and has attempted to give, very briefly, some idea of the organs of sense, commencing in each case with those of man himsi'lf. THE PLEASURES OF LIFE. 12mo. Cloth, 50 cents ; paper, 25 cents. CONTEXTS.— Tut: Duty op Happiness. The Happiness or Duty. A Song of Books. The Choice op Books. The Blessing op Friends. The VALtTE OF Time. The Pleasures of Travel. The Pleasures of Home, Science. Education. New York: D. APPLETON & CO., 1, 3, & 5 Bond Street, D. APPLETON & CO.'S PUBLICATIONS. JOHN TYNDALL'S WORKS. ESSAYS O^ THE FLOATING MATTER OF THE AIR, in Relation to rutrefaetion and Infection, 12mo. Cloth, ^1.50. ON FOR>IS OF WATER, in Clouds, Kivers, Ice, and Glaciers. With ys Illustrations, l^rao. Cloth, $1.50. HEAT AS A MODE OF MOTION. New edition. 12mo. Cloth, $2.50. ON SOUND : A Course of Eight Lectures delivered at the Royal Institution of Great Britain. Illustrated. 12mo. New edition. Cloth, $2.00. FRAGMENTS OF SCIENCE FOR UNSCIENTIFIC PEO- PLE. 12mo. New revised and enlarged edition. Cloth, ^2.50. LIGHT AND ELECTRICITY. 12mo. Cloth, $1.25. LESSONS IN ELECTRICITY, lSV5-'76. 12mo. Cloth, $1.00. HOURS OF EXERCISE IN THE ALPS. With Illustrations. 12nio. Cloth, $2.00. FARADAY AS A DISCOVERER. A Memoir. 12mo. Cloth, $1.00. CONTRIBUTIONS TO MOLECULAR PHYSICS in the Do- main of Radiant Heat. $5.00, SIX LECTURES ON LIGHT. Delivered in America in 1872- '73. With an Appendix and numerous Illustrations. Cloth, $1.50. FAREWELL BANQUET given at Delmonico's, New York. Paper, 50 cents. ADDRESS delivered before the British Association, assembled at Bel- fast. Revised, with Additions. 12mo. Paper, 50 cents. RESEARCHES ON DIAMAGNETISM AND MAGNE- CRYSTALLIC ACTION, including the Question of Diamag- netic Polarity. With Ten Plates. 12mo. Cloth, $1,50. NEW FRAGMENTS. 12mo. Cloth, $2.00. New York: D. APPLETON & CO., 1, 3, & 5 Bond Street. D. APPLETON & CO/S PUBLICATIONS. CHARLES DARWIN'S WORKS. ORIGIN OF SPECIES BY MEANS OF NATURAI. SELECTION, OR THE PRESERVATION OF FA- VORED RACES IN THE STRUGGLE FOR LIFE. From sixth and last London edition. 2 vols., 12mo. Cloth, $4.00. DESCENT OF MAN, AND SELECTION IN RELATION TO SEX. With many Illustrations. A new edition. 12ino. Cloth, $3.00. A NATURALIST'S VOYAGE AROUND THE WORLD. Journal of Researches into the Natural History and Geology of Countries visited during the Voyage of H. M. S. " Beagle." Illustrated with Maps and 100 Views of the places visited and described, chiefly from sketches taken on the spot, by R. T. Pritchett. 8vo. Cloth, $5.00. Also popular edition. 12mo. Cloth, $2.00. THE STRUCTURE AND DISTRIBUTION OF CORAL REEFS. Based on Observations made during the Voyage of the "Beagle." With Charts and Illustrations. 12mo. Cloth, $2.00. GEOLOGICAL OBSERVATIONS on the Volcanic Islands and Parts of South America visited during the Voyage of the '= Beagle." With Maps and Illustrations. 12mo. Cloth, $2.50. EMOTIONAL EXPRESSIONS OF MAN AND THE LOWER ANIMALS. 12mo. Cloth, $3.50, D. APPLE TON & 00.' S PUBUOATIONS. Charles Darwin's Works. — {Continued.) THE VARIATIONS OF ANIMALS AND PLANTS UNDER DOMESTICATION. With a Preface, by Professor Asa Gray, 2 vols. Illustrated. Cloth, $5.00. INSECTIVOROUS PLANTS. 12mo. Cloth, $2.00. MOVEMENTS AND HABITS OF CLIMBING PLANTS. With Illustrations. 12mo. Cloth, $1.25. THE VARIOUS CONTRIVANCES BY WHICH ORCHIDS ARE FERTILIZED BY INSECTS. Revised edition, with Illustrations. 12mo. Cloth, $1.75. THE EFFECTS OF CROSS AND SELF FERTILIZA- TION IN THE VEGETABLE KINGDOM. 12mo. Cloth, $2.00. DIFFERENT FORMS OF FLOWERS ON PLANTS OF THE SAME SPECIES. With Illustrations. 12mo. Cloth, $1.50. THE POWER OF MOVEMENT IN PLANTS. By Charles Darwin, LL. D., F. R. S., assisted by Francis Darwin. With Illustrations. 12mo. Cloth, $2.00. THE FORMATION OF VEGETABLE MOULD THROUGH THE ACTION OF WORMS. With Observations on their Habits. With Illustrations. 12mo. Cloth, $1.50. New York: D. APPLETON & CO., 1, 3, & 5 Bond Street. D. APPLETON & CO/S PUBLICATION'S. DR. HENRY MAUDSLEY'S WORKS. KODY AND WILL: Being an Essay concerning Will in He Metaphysical, Physiological, and Pathological Aspects. 12mcv, Cloth, $2.50. POr>Y AND MIND : An Inquiry into their Connection and Mutual Influence, specially in reference to Mental Lisorders. 1 vol., 12mo^ Cloth, $1.50. PHYSIOLOGY AND PATHOLOGY OF MIND: PHYSIOLOGY OF THE MIND. New edition. 1 vol., 12mo Cloth, $2.00. Contents: Chapter I. On the Method of the Study of the Mind. — II. The Mind and the Nervous System. — III. The Spinal Cord, or Tertiary Nervous Centres ; or. Nervous Centres of Reflex Action. — IV. Secondary Nervous Centres ; or. Sensory Ganglia ; Sensorium Commune. — V. Hemispherical Ganglia ; Cortical Cells of the Cerebral Hemispheres; Ideational Nervous Centres, Primary Nervous Centres; Intellectorium Commune. — VI. The Emotions. — VII. Volition. — VIII. — Motor Nervous Centres, or Mo- torium Commune and Actuation or Effection. — IX. Memory and Imagination. PATHOLOGY OF THE MIND. Being the Third Edition of the Second Part of the "Physiology and Pathology of Mind," recast, enlarged, and rewritten. 1 vol., Vlmo. Cloth, $2.00. Con- TEXTS : Chapter I. Sleep and Dreaming. — II. Hypnotism, Somnam- bulism, and Allied States. — III. The Causation and Pi^evention of Insanity: (A) Etiologicil. — IV. The same continued — V. The Causation and Prevention of Insanity : (B) Pathological. — VI. The Insanity of Early Life. — VII. The Symptomatology of Insanity. — ■ VIII. The same continued. — IX. Clinical Groups of Mental Dis- ease.— X. The Morbid Anatomy of Mental Derail u'cmcnt. — XI. The Treatment of Menta! Disorders. RESPONSIBILITY IN MENTAL DISEA'SE. (International Scientific Series.) 1 vol., 12mo. Cloth, $1.50. "The author is at home in his subject, and presents his views in nn almost siupu = larly clear and satisfactory manner. . . . The vohinie is a v.duable tontrib'ition to one of the most difficult and at the same time one of the most important subjects of in- vestig-ation at the present Any .""—New York Observer. " Handles the important topic with masterly power, and its sui^g-sstions are practi- cal and of great value." — Providence Press. New York : D. APPLETON & CO., 1, 3, & 5 Bjnd Street. D. APPLETON & CO.'S PUBLICATIONS. GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLES AND NERVES. l}y Dr. I. KosKNTHAL, Prcfossor of Physiology at the University of Erlangen. With seventy-live Woodcuts. (" International Scientitic Series.") 12nio. Cloth, $1.50. " The attempt, at a connected account of the general physiology of niuecles and nerves is, as far as I know, the first of its kind. The general data for this branch of science have been gained only within the past thirty years.'''— Uxtract Jro?n Preface. SIGHT : An Exposition of the Principles of Monocular and Binocular Vision. I>y Joseph Le Conte, LL. D., author of "Elements of Ge- ology"; "Religion and Science"; and Professor of Geology and Natural History in the University of California. With numerous Illustrations. i2mo. Cloth, $1.50. " It is pleasant to find an American book which can rank with the very best of foreign works on thit^ subject. Professor Le Conte has long been known as an original investigator in this department ; all that he gives us is treated with a master-hand."— 2'/ie Nation. ANIMAL LIFE, as affected by the Natural Conditions of Existence. By Karl Semper, Professor of the University of Wiirzburg. With 2 'Maps and 106 Woodcuts, and Index. 12mo. Cloth, $2.00. " Tliis is in many respects one of the most interesting contributions to zoological literature which has appeared for some iimQ."— Nature. THE ATOMIC THEORY. By Ad. W^urtz, Membra de I'Institut; Doyen llonoraire de la Eaeulte de Medecine ; Professeur h. la Faculte des Sciences de Paris. Translated by E. Cleminshaw, M. A., F. C. S., F.I. C, Assistant Master at Sherborne School. 12mo. Cloth, $1.50. "There was need for a book like this, which discusses the atomic theory both in its historic evolution and in its present form. And perhaps no man of this age could have been selected so al)le to perform the task in a masterly way as the illustrious French chemist, Adolph Wurtz. It is impossible to convey to the reader, in a notice like this, any adequate idea of the scope, lucid instructiVeness, and scientific interest of Professor VVurtz's book. The modern problems of chemistry, which are commonly so obscure from imperfect exposition, are here made wonderfully clear and attractive." — The Popular Science Monthly . THE CRAYFISH. An Introduction to the Study of Zoology. Bv Professor T. H. Huxley, F. K. S. With 82 Illustrations. ' 12mo. Cloth, $1.75. "Whoever will follow these pages, crayfish in hand, and will try to verify for himself the statements which they contain, will find himself brouiiht face to face with all the great zoological questions which excite so lively an interest at the present day." "The reader of this valuable monograph will lay it down with a feelins: of wonder at the amount and variety of matter which has been got out of so seem- ingly slight and inipretcnding a mhiccX."— Saturday Review. New York: D. APPLETON" & CO., 1, 3, & 5 Bond Street. D. APPLETON & CO.'S PUBLICATIONS. OUTINGS AT ODD TIMES. By Charles C. Abbott, author of " Days out of Doors " and " A Naturalist's Rambles about Home." i6mo. Cloth, gilt top, $1.25. " A charming little volume, literally alone with Nature, for it discusses seasons and the fields, birds, etc., with the loving freedom of a naturalist born. Every page reads like a sylvan poem; and for the lovers of the beautiful in quiet out door and out-of- town life, this beautifully bound and attractively printed little volume will prove a companion and friend." — Rochester Union and Advertiser. A NA TURALIST'S RAMBLES ABO UT HOME. By Charles C. Abbott. i2mo. Cloth, $1.50. " The home about which Dr. Abbott rambles is clearly the haunt of fowl and fish, of animal and insect life ; and it is of the habits and nature of these that he discourses pleasantly in this book. Summer and winter, morning and evening, he has been in thi open air all the time on the alert for some new revelation of instinct, or feeling, or character on the part of his neighbor creatures. Most that he sees and hears he reporis agreeably to us, as it was no doubt delightful to hmiself. Books like this, which are free from all the technicalities of science, but yet lack little that has scien- tific value, are well suited to the reading of the young. I'heir atmosphere is a healthy o.ie for boys in particular to breathe. " — Boston 'I ranscript. A YS OUT OF DOORS. By Charles C. Abbott- i2mo. Cloth, $1.50. "'Days out of Doors' is a series of sketches of animal life by Charles C. Abbott, a naturalist whose graceful writings have entertained and instructed the public before now. The essays and narratives in this book are grouped in twelve chapters, named after the months of the year. Under ' January ' the author talks of squirrels, musk- rats, water-snakes, and the predatory animals that withstand the rigor of winter; under 'February' of fro^s and herons, crows and blackbirds; under 'March' of gulls and fishes and foxy spat rows: and so on appropriately, instructively, and divertingly through the whJe twelve " — }\eiv York Snn. D T HE FLA YTIME NA TURALIST. By Dr. J. E. Taylor, F. L. S., editor of " Science Gossip." With 366 Illus- trations. i2mo. Cloth, $1.50. "The work contains abund.nnt evidence of the author's knowledge and enthusiasm, and any boy who may rea 1 it caiefully is sure to find something to attract him. The style is clear and lively, and tnere are many good illustrations." — Nature. n^HE ORIGIN OF FLORAL STRUCTURES ' through Insects and other Agencies. By the Rev. George Henslow, Professor of Botany, Queen's College. With nu- merous Illustrations. i2mo. Cloth, $1.75. "Much has been written on the structure of flowers, and it might seem almost superfluous to attempt to say anything m re on the subject, but it is only withm the last few years that a new literature has sprung up, in which the authors have described their observations and given their interpretations of the uses of floral mechanisms, more especially in connection with the processes of fertilization." — From Introduction. New York: D. APPLETON & CO., i, 3, & 5 Bond Street. D. APPLETON & CO.'S PUBLICATIONS. T 'HE GARDEN'S STORY ; or, Pleasures and Trials of an Amatctir Gardener. By George H. Ellwanger. \\'ith Head and Tail Pieces by Rhead. i2mo. Cloth, extra, $1.50. "Mr. Ellwanger's instinct rarely errs in matters of taste. He writes out of the fullness of experimental knowledge, but his knowledge differs from that of many a trained cultivator in that his skill in garden practice is guided by a refined aesthetic sensibility, and his appreciation of what is beautiful in nature is healthy, hearty, and catholic. His record of the garden year, as we have said, begins with the earliest violet, and it follows the season through until the witch-hazel is blossomii.g on the border of the wintry woods. . . . This little book can not fail to give pleasure to all who take a genuine interest in rural life."— AVw York Tribune. r HE ORIGIN OE CULTIVATED PLANTS. By Alphonse de Candolle. i2mo. Cloth, $2.00. "Though a fact farniliar to botanists, it is not generally known how great is the uncertainty as to the origin of many of the most important cultivated plants. ... In endeavoring to unravel the matter, a knowledge of b( tany, of geography, of geology, of history, and of philosophy is required. By a combination of testimony derived from these sources M. de Candolle has been enpbled to deterinine the botanical origin and geographical source of the large proportion of si ecies he deals with." — The A thetitpuin. T HE EOLK-LORE OE PIANTS. By T. F. This- ELTON Dyer, M. A. 121110. Cloth, $1.50. "A handsome and deeply interesting volume. ... In all respects the book is ex- cellent. Its airangement is simple and intelligible, its style bright and alluring. . . . To all who seek an introduction to one of the most attracti\e branches of folk- lore, this delightful volume may be warmly commended. — Notes attd Qjteries. F LOWERS AND THEIR PEDIGREES. By Grant Ai.len, author of "Vignettes of Nature," etc. Illus- trated. i2mo. Cloth, $1.50. "No writer treats scientific subjects with so much esse and charm of style as Mr. Grant Allen. The study is a delightful one, and the book is fascinating to any one who has either love for flowers or curiosity about them." — Ha7-tJord Courarif. "Any one with even a smattering of botanical knowledge, and with either a heart or mind, must be charmed with this collection of essays." — Chicago Evening yoztrnal. y^HE GEOLOGICAL HL STORY OE PLANTS. -* By Sir J. William Dawson, F. R. S. Illustrated. i2mo. Cloth, $1.75. "The object of this work is to give, in a connected form, a summary of the develoD- ment of the vegetable kingdom in geological time. To the geologist and lotanist the subject is one of importance with reference to their special pursuits, and one on which it has not been easy to find any convenient manual of information. It is hoped that its treatment in the present volume will also be found sufficiently simple and popular to be attractive to the general reader." — Frojn the Freface. New York : D. APPLETON & CO., i, 3, & 5 Bond Street. T D, APPLETON & CO.'S PUBLICATIONS. 'HE DOG IN HEALTH AND IN DISEASE. By Wesley Mills, M. D., D. V. S., author of " A Text-Book of Animal Physiology," "A Text-Book of Comparative Physiol- ogy," etc. With colored plate, 38 full-page cuts, and numerous other Illustrations. i2mo. Cloth, $2.25. The author of this work has undertaken, in a clear, concise, unlechnical way, to supply the large class of intelligent dog owners and breeders, and veterinarians, with the information necessary for the proper care, manage- ment, and treatment of the dog. His well-known reputation as a writer and lecturer on human and veterinary physiology, his special study of canine diseases, and his long experience as a breeder of dogs, insure a thorough and correct handling of the subject. " The library of every one interested in the dog should contain a copy of this work." — A »iericnn Stock-keeper, Boston. "The numerous illustrations in the book have been drawn from various sources, especial pains having been taken to furnish models for judging the various breeds of dogs in the full-page plates." — Montreal IVitness. " Altogether the work is one of great use to the breeder and the veterinary student, and one that should find a place in every dog-man's kennel-shelf and Whrary."— Forest and Stream. " Very interesting and valuable." — New York Times. " A practical protest against the treatment of dogs according to the light of the horse-doctor. The book is intended for all persons who breed, keep, or in any way take a special interest in the dog. . . . One half the book is devoted to the diseases of the dog. The symptoms and treatment ars carefully given, and there is added a tabic of doses of the drugs found most efficacious. The volume is one to be cordially recom- mended."— Philadelphia hiquirer. y^HE COMPARATIVE ANATOMY OF THE ^ DOMESTICA TED ANIMALS. By A. Chauveau, M. D., LL. D., Inspector General of Veterinary Schools in France. Second English edition, translated and edited by George Flem- ing, C. B., F. R. C. V. S., etc., Examiner in Anatomy for the Royal College of Veterinary Surgeons, etc. With 585 Illustrations. 1084 pages. 8vo. Cloth, $7.00. "This work has long since earned for itself the position of foremost rank in its particular field, and, indeed, stands without a rival in completeness, scientific arrange- ment, accurate detail, and practical adaptability to the necessities of the veteiinarj' ,-ind to the student of general anatomy. It is profusely illustrated and handsomely printed." — Medical Record. " 'Chauveau's Anatomy,' as it now stands, is easily first, as it is, in fact, indispen- sable. The reputation of its author and editor is sufficient to prove that this work is all that it claims to be, and it may now continue to be accepted as in every way worthy of the position accorded to it as the best on the subject." — Therapeutic Gazette. " Invaluable to all who desire a scientific knowledge of the animals utilized by man in the performance of his work. All intelligent persons interested in animals will greatly enjoy the study of the volume. As already intimated, it is issued in the elegant style characteristic of Appletons' publications " — American Lancet. New York : D. APPLETON & CO., i, 3, & 5 Bond Street. T D. APPLETON & CO.'S PUBLICATIONS. WORKS BY ARABELLA B. BUCKLEY (MRS. FISHER). HE FAIRY-LAND OF SCIENCE. With 74 II- T lustrations. Cloth, gilt, $1.50. " Deserves to take a perir.anent place in the literature o( yonih."— Lrndon Times. "So interesting tliat, having once opened the book, we do not know how to leave off reading." — SaUirday Review. H ROUGH MAGIC GLASSES and other Lectures. A Sequel to " The Fairy-Land of Science." Cloth, $1.50. CONTENTS. The Magician's Chamber by Moon- An Hour with the Sun. LIGHT. An Evening with the Stars. MagicGlasses AND HowtoUseThem. Little Beings from a Miniature Fairy Rings and How They are Made. Ocean. The Life-History of Lichens and The Dartmoor Ponies. Mosses. The Magician's Dream of Ancient The Hi.story of a Lav.\-Stream. Days. L IFF AND HER CHIIDREN : Glhnpses of Ani- mal Life from the Amoeba to tJie Instcts. W ith over 100 Illus- trations. Cloth, gilt, $1.50. " The work forms a charming introduction to the study cf zoology — the science of living things — which, we trust, will find its way into many hands." — Nature. TJ^INNERS IN LIFE'S RACE ; or, 7 he Great ^^ Backboned Family. With numerous Illustrations. Cloth, gilt, $L5o. " We can conceive no better gift-book than this volume. Miss Bi:ckley has spared no pains to incorporate in her b lok tlie latest results of scientific research. The illus- trations in the book deserve the highest praise — they are numerous, accurate, and striking. ' — Spectator. /I SHORT HISTORY OF NATURAL SCI- ■^ ■*■ ENCE ; and of the Progress of Discovery from the Time of the Greeks to the Present Time. New edition, revised and re- arranged. With 77 Illustrations. Cloth, $2.00. •'The work, though mainly intended for children and young persons, may be ircst advantageously read by many persons of riper are, and may serve to implant in their minds a fuller and clearer conception of * the promises, the achievements, and claims of science.' " — Jottrnal of Science. M. ORAL TEACHINGS OF SCIENCE. Cloth, 75 cents. "The book is intended for readers v\ ho would not fake up an elaborate philo- sophical work — those who, feelin.tr puzz'ed and adrift in the present cIkk s of opinion, may welcome even a partial solution, from a scientific point of view, of the difficulties which oppress their minds." — From the Preface. New York: D. APPLETON & CO.. i, 3, cK; 5 Bond Street. D. APPLETON & CO.'S PUBLICATIONS. T HE ICE AGE IN NORTH AMERICA, and its Bearings upon the Antiquity of Man. By G. Frederick Wright, D. D., LL. D., F. G. S.A., Professor in Oberlin Theological Seminary ; Assistant on the United States Geo- logical Survey. With an appendix on "The Probable Cause of Glaciation," by Warren Uppiam, F. G. S.A., Assistant on the Geological Surveys of New Hampshire, Minnesota, and the United States. New and enlarged edition. With 150 Maps and Illustrations. 8vo, 625 pages, and Index. Cloth, $5.00. •' Not a novel in all the list of this year's publications has in it any pages of more thrilling interest than can be found in this book by Professor Wright. There is noth- ing pedantic in the narrative, and the most serious themes and startling discoveries are treated with such charming naturalness and simplicity that boys and girls, as well as their seniors, will be attracted to the story, and find it difficult to lay it aside, —hew York Journal 0/ Co7niuerce. "One of the most absorbing and interesting of all the recent issues in the depart- ment of popular science." — Chicago Herald. "Though his subject is a very deep one, his style is so very unaffected and per- spicuous that even the unscientific reader can peruse it with intelligence and profit, .n reading such a book we are led almost to wonder that so much that is scientihc can be put in language so comparatively simple." — New York Observer. "The author has seen with his own eyes the most important phenomena of the Ice age on this continent from Alaine to Alaska. In the work itself, elementary description is combined with a broad, scientific, and philosophic method, without abandoning for a moment the purely scientific character. Professor Wright has contrived to give tlie whole a philosophical direction which lends interest and inspiration to it, and which in the chapters on Man and the Glacial Period rises to something like dramatic intensity." — The Indepcndefit. ". . . To the great advance that has been made in late years in the accuracy and cheapness of processes of photographic reproduction is due a further signal advantage that Dr. Wright's work possesses over his predecessors'. He has thus been able to illustrate most of the natural phenomena to which he refers by views taken in the field, many of which have been genciously loaned by the United States Geological Survey, in some rases from unpublished material ; and he has admirably supplemented them by numerous maps and diagrams." — 7 he Nation. M AN AND THE GLACIAL PERIOD. By G. Frederick Wright, D. D., LL. D., author of "The Ice Age in North America," " Logic of Christian Evidences," etc. No. 69, International Scientific Series. With numerous Illus- trations. i2mo. Cloth, $1.75. Every one is interested in ancestry, yet the roots of family trees have not struck down to the Glacial period, and we are left to wonder regarding the manners and customs of our ancestors in the remote age of ice. Who and what these ancestors were, is told us in simple, entertaining, popular style by Professor Wright, and his fascinating narrative is re-enforced by a multi- tude of illustrations. New York : D. APPLETON & CO., i, 3, & 5 Bond Street. E D. APPLETON & CO.'S PUBLICATIONS. SSAYS UPON SOME CONTROVERTED QUESTIONS. By Thomas li. Huxley, F. R. S., author of " Man's Place in Nature," etc. i2mo. Cloth, $2.00. " Professor Huxley is one of the most vigorous and uncompromising polemics of tlie age. He is admittedly also one of the very ablest. In tliat debatable land which touches the confines of science on the one hand and the confines of religion on the other, there is no living man who can be called his supeiior. Professor Huxley takes n*) mysterious or incomprehensible position. He plants himself on facts. There is, he contends, no other safe or solid position. . . . He has compelled attention to the truths of scienje. He has made religion more intelligent. He lias helped us to see that science and religion are not mutually destructive — that the God of Nature and the God of the Bible are one God." — Ckristmn at Work. ' . . . Mr. Hu.\ley's literary style, also, is singularly lucid, polished, graceful, and strong. There is no living writer of English more 'cunning offence' in dialectics; none who has a better gift for clothing his ideas in perspicuous and elegant language. He may be an agnostic or an infidel, with all that is implied in the words; but his can- did declarations are always uttered with the refinement of a gentleman and the clear- ness of a tUoroiigh scholar." — I Jiiladelphia BiiLetiyi. T 'HE NATURALIST IN LA PLATA. By C. H. Hudson, C. M. Z. S., joint author of " Argentine Ornithol- ogy." With 27 Illustrations. 8vo, 388 pages. Cloth, $4.00. " Mr. Hudson is not only a clever naturalist, but he po.ssesses the rare gift of in- teresting his readers in whatever attracts him, and of being dissatisfied with mere ob- servation unless it enables him to philosophize as well. With his lucid accounts of bird, beast, and insect, no one will fail to be delighted." — Loiidoti Academy. " Mr. Hudson is a scientist, it is true, but apparently not enough of a scientist to have lost the faculty of wonder and the knack of making readers wonder with him. He has worked on an ornithology of the Argentine States, but his case appears to be that of Audubon, and the results of avoiding the drudgery of science the same. At least, he has so many new and interesting and piquant things to tell about the insects, birds, and beasts found on the pampas, that few books of the sort, if any, which have been published smce the work of Belt appeared, can vie with it in charm." — New York Times. NEW EDITION OF RAGMEN TS OF SCIENCE. By John Tyndall, F. R.S., author of "Sound," "Heat as a Mode of Motion," etc. Revised and enlarged edition. 2 vols. i2mo. Cloth, $4.00. The first edition of Professor Tyndall's "Fragments of Science" was published some twenty years ago as a single volume, which was made up of a score or more of his detached essays, addresses, and reviews. The book was afterward revised, some of the papers recast, and from time to time new ones added until, the size of the work becoming somewhat unwieldy, the present two-volume edition was decided upon. This contains fifteen addi- tional papers, and represents the author's latest changes and revisions. The volumes are uniform with " New Fragments, recently issued, and the three together include all the occasional writings which their author has decided to preserve in permanent form. F New York . D. APPLETON & CO., i, 3, & 5 Bond Street. E D. APPLETON & CO.'S PUBLICATIONS. VOLUTION IN SCIENCE, PHILOSOPHY, AND ART. A Series of Seventeen Lectures and Discussions before the Brooklyn Ethical Association. With 3 Portraits. 466 pages. i2iriO. Cloth, $2.00. Separate Lectures, in pam= phlet form, 10 cents each. These popular essays, by some of the ablest exponents of the doctrine of evolution in this country, will be read with pleasure and profit by all lovers of good literature and suggestive thought. The principle of evolution, being universal, admits of a great diversity of applications and illustrations ; some of those appearing in the present volume are distinctively fresh and new. CONTENTS. 1. Alfred Russel Wallace By Edward D. Cope, Ph. D. 2. Ernst Haeckel By Thaddeus B. Wakeman. 3. The Scientific Method By Francis E. Abbot, Ph.D. 4. Herbert Spencer's Synthetic Philosophy. By Benj. F. Underwood. ^. Evolution of Chemistry By Robert G. EccLES, M. D. 6. Evolution of Electric and Magnetic Physics. By Arthur E. Kennelly. 'J. Evolution of Botany By Fred J. Wulling, Ph. G. 8. Zoology as related to Evolution ... By Rev. John C. Kimball. 9. Form and Color in Nature .... By William Potts. 10. Optics as related to Evolution . . . By L. A. W. Alleman, M. D. 11. Evolution of Art By John A. Taylor. Y-z. Evolution of Architecture EyRev. John W. Chadwick. XT,. Evolution of Sculpture By Prof. Thomas Davidson. 14. Evolution of Painting By Forrest P. Rundell. 15. Evolution of Mtisic By Z. Sidney Sampson. 16. Life as a Fine Art By Lewis G. Janes, M. D. 17. The Doctrine of Evolutiofi : its Scope and Influence. By Prof. John Fiske. "A valuable series." — Chicago Evening fournal. 'The addresses include some of the most important presentations and epitomes pii?> lished in America. They are all upon important subjects, are prepared with preatcare, and are delivered for the most part by highly eminent authorities " — Public Opiuiou. " As a popular exposition of the latest phases of evolution this series is thorough and authoritative. " — Cincinnati Times-Star. New York : D. APPLETON & CO., i, 3, & 5 Bond Street. N- D. APPLETON /?: CO.'S PUBLICATIONS. £IV FRAGMENTS. By John Tyndall, F. R. S., author of " Fragments of Science," " Heat as a Mode of Motion," etc. i2mo. 500 pages. Cloth, $2.00. Among: the subjects treated in this volume are "The Sabbath," " Life in the Alps," "The Rainbow and its Congeners," "Common Water," and "Atoms, Molecules, and Ether- Waves." In addition to the popular treat- ment of scientific themes, the author devotes several chapters to biographical sketches of the utmost interest, including studies of Count Rumford and Thomas Young, and chapters on " Louis Pasteur, his Life and Labors," and " Personal Recollections of Thomas Carlyle." "Tyndall is the happiest combination of the lover of nature and the lover of science, and these fragments are admirable examples of his delightful style, and pioofs of his comprehensive intellect " — 1 kiladclphia Evening Bulletm. "The name of this illustrious scientist and lUleraieur\% known wherever the Eng- lish language is the mother tongue, or is even freely spoken. Whatever he does or says comes with a stamp of authority as from one who speaks with power, knowing whereof he affirms. He is able and effective, both as a talker and writer, as scientist or teacher. I'o those who know anything of Prof. Tyndall's life and labors, scientific or literary, it is superfluous to say that his utterances bring his hearers or readers face to face with the latest knowledge on the subject he discusses." — New York Commer- cial A dvertiser. M M^- ORAL TEACHINGS OF SCIENCE. By Ara- bella B, Buckley, author of "The Fairy-Land of Science," " Life and her Children," etc. i2mo. Cloth, 75 cents. " The book is intended for readers who would not take up an elaborate philosophical work — those who, feeling puzzled and adrift in the present chaos of opinion, may welcome even a partial solution, from a scientific point of view, of the difficulties which oppress their minds." — Frotn the Preface. ''AX MULLER AND THE SCIENCE OF -^^^ LANGUAGE. A Criticism. By William Dwight Whitney, Professor in Yale University. i2mo. 79 pages. Paper cover, 50 cents. This critique relates to the new edition of Prof. Muller's well-known work on Language. " For many," says Prof. Whitney, in his preface, " the book has been their first introduction to linguistic study ; and doubtless to a large proportion of English-speaking readers, especially, it is still the principal and most authoritative text-book of that study, as regards both methods and re- sults. A work holding such a position calls for careful criticism, that it may not be trusted where it is untrustworthy, and so do harm to the science which it was intended to help." "This caustic review of Max IMiiller's latest edition of his 'Science of Language* will command attention for more and higher merits than its brilliant criticism. It up- holds a theory of language and of its development which, though not taught by Max Miiller, is held by the great masters of linguistic science. The reader not versed in the science, nor well read in its controversial literature, will get from this brochure a con- ception of the critical points of the subject which he might miss in the reading of many larger and more systematic treatises."— 7^,^^ Jndependent, New York. New York : D. APPLETON & CO., i, 3, & 5 Bond Street. D. APPLETON & CO.'S PUBLICATIONS. MODERN SCIENCE SERIES. Edited by Sir John Lubbock, Bart., F. R S. The works to be comprised in the " Modern Science Series" are primarily not for the student, nor for the young, but for the educated layman who needs to know the present state and result o! scientific investigation, and who has neither time nor inclina- tion to become a specialist on the subject which arouses his interest. Each book will be complete in itself, and, while thoroughly scientific in treatment, its subject will as far as possible be j^resented in language divested of needless technicalities. Illustra- tions will be given wheiever needed by the text. The following are the volumes thus far issued. Others are in preparation. T HE CA USE OE AN ICE AGE. By Sir Robert Ball, LL. D., F. R. S., Royal Astronomer of Ireland, author of "Starland." i2mo. Cloth, $i.oo. " Sir Robert Ball's book is, as a matter of course, admirably written. Though but a small one, it is a most important contribution to geology." — Lofidon Saturday Review. " A fascinating subject, cleverly related and almost colloquially discussed." — Phila- delphia Public Ledger. 7^HE HORSE: A Study in Natural History. By William H. Flower, C. B., Director in the British Natural History Museum. With 27 Illustrations. i2mo. Cloth, $1.00. " The author admits that there are 3,800 separate treatises on the horse already pub- lished, but he thinks that he can add something to the amount of useful information now before the public, and that something not heretofore written will be found in this book. The volume gives a large amount of information, both scientific and practical, on the noble animal of which it treats." — New York Coi7i})tercial Advertiser. T HE OAK: A Study in Botany. By H. Marshall Ward, F. R. S. With 53 Illustrations. i2mo. Cloth, $i.co. " \n excellent volume for young persons with a taste for scientific studies, becacre it will lead them from the contemplation of superficial appearances and those gcnei alities which arc so misleading to the immature mind, to a consideration of the methods of systematic investigation." — Boston Beacofi. " From the acorn to the timber which has figured so gloriously in English ships and houses, the tree is fully described, and all its living and preserved be.tuties ; r.d virtues, in nature and in construction, are recounted and pictured." — Brooklyn Ea^ie. PTHNOLOGY IN EOIKLORE. By George -^— ^ Lawrence Gomme, F. S. A., President of the Folklore Society, etc. i2mo. Cloth, $1.00. This book is an attempt to ascertain and set forth the principles upon which folklore may be classified, in order to arrive at some of the results which should follow its study, giving the subject the importance it deserves in connection with researches in ethnology. New York: D. APPLETON & CO., r, 3, c^- 5 Bond Street. OL377.S4 R65 1893 hM^ hsi, s(,„ ),sl, ,n.| Harvard MCZ Library 3 2044 062 441 613