'^ tsk o ^ 1— 1 PQ M W H CD ^ Ul • O P^ a o CT} _j W H < X a. z o o o UJ I I- 00 -i -1 UJ UJ a o (E z D LIGHT AND THE BEHAVIOR OF ORGANISMS BY S. O. MAST, Ph.D. ASSOCIATE PROFESSOR OF BIOLOGY, GOUCHER COLLEGE, JOHNSTON RESEARCH SCHOLAR, JOHNS HOPKINS UNIVERSITY (1907-1908) FIRST EDITION FIRST THOUSAND NEW YORK JOHN WILEY & SONS London : CHAPMAN & HALL, Limited 1911 Copyright, igio BY S. O. MAST Stanbopc iPress F. H. CrUSON COMPANY BOSTON, U. S A r PREFACE Parts I and II of this volume consist of an essay for -hich the Cartwright Prize was awarded by the College f Physicians and Surgeons of Columbia University in 909. The entire volume is the outgrowth of an intensive and extensive study of the processes of orientation in plants and animals, especially those without eyes, i.e., a study of he perplexing and interesting question as to how these or- canisms regulate their activities so as to bend or move oward or from the source of stimulation. But while the >ook deals primarily with the question of orientation, it las a broader aspect and may be considered a treatise on the behavior of organisms based on their reactions to light, 'he generality of the treatment of the subject of actions 1 organisms, including plants as well as animals, it is hoped all make the work of value to all students of nature, espe- cially to those interested in comparative psychology, zoology, botany and physiology. Throughout the work it has been my aim first of all to tate precisely what organisms do under different condi- tions of illumination, and then to consider the bearing of the observed reactions on the various theories that have been formulated regarding reactions in general. This aim has made it necessary to present somewhat lengthy and detailed descriptions of methods of stimulation and re- sponses which, it is feared, may be rather tedious to those who are interested only in the general aspect of the problem. To such it will be of particular advantage to consult freely the table of contents and the summaries. The historical chapters which are found in Part I deal with the origin and development of theories regarding the activities of organisms, especially those associated with light. No attempt has been made in these chapters to y • • • ^ 111 iv PREFACE review all the literature on behavior. Only such works are here referred to as appear to have a theoretical bear- ing, but many others are considered elsewhere. Part II is devoted largely to the description and dis- cussion of experimental observations on orientation made by the author during the past five years. Only a few of these have been previously published. The remaining parts of the book are more general and contain relatively much less original matter. A large part of the experimental work connected with this volume was done at Johns Hopkins University dur- ing my residence as Johnston Research Scholar. To this institution I am greatly indebted, not only for the scholar- ship, but also for exceptional facilities placed at my com- mand by the late Professor W. K. Brooks, Director of the Zoological Laboratory during my residence, and for friendly courtesies extended on every hand by other members of the University. I am also under obligation to the Marine Biological Laboratory of Woods Hole, Massachusetts, for research facilities during the summer of 1907, and to the United States Bureau of Fisheries for similar privileges during the following two summers, and especially to the Director of the Laboratory of the Bureau of Fisheries at Woods Hole, Massachusetts, Doctor F. B. Sumner, for generously supplying my needs. It is a pleasure to ac- knowledge my further indebtedness to Professor H. S. Jennings for his enthusiastic interest and support in the work at all times and for critically reading the manuscript; to Professors G. H. Parker and J. B. Watson for valuable suggestions after reading much of the work in manuscript; to Professor R. M. Yerkes for his thorough criticism re- garding both composition and contents; and to my wife, Grace Tennent Mast, for invaluable literary aid and criti- cism. The author however must be held responsible for all of the subject-matter. ^ ^ ,^ Samuel Ottmar Mast. Baltimore, Maryland, February 4, 1910. TABLE OF CONTENTS PART I INTRODUCTION AND HISTORICAL REVIEW CHAPTER I General Introduction CHAPTER n Historical Review Concerning the Origin and Development of Ideas and Theories Regarding Movements in Plants and Animals WITH Special Reference to the Qu-estion of Tropisms PAGE 1. Early Investigations and Ideas concerning Movement in Or- ganisms S 2. First Attempts at Mechanical Explanation of Life Phenomena — Galen, Harvey, Descartes, BoreUi, Ray 6 3. Period of VitaHsm 8 4. Return to Mechanical Explanations — Johannes Miiller, De Candolle 8 5. Evolution and its Effect on the Study of Behavior in Plants and Animals — Darwin, Bert, Graber, Romanes, Lubbock, Preyer 9 6. Introduction of the Term " Tropism " and Development of its Apphcation to Different Reactions — De Candolle, Knight, Frank, Hofmeister, Darwin 11 7. Further Analysis of Reactions in Plants to Light — Sachs, Strasburger, Engelmann, Darwin 13 CHAPTER III Historical Review Concerning the Origin and Development of Ideas and Theories Regarding Movements in Plants and Animals with Special Reference to the Question of Tropisms (continued) I, The Application of the Underlying Principle of Tropisms in the Study of Animal Behavior as opposed to this Study from the Point of View of Comparative Psychology — Loeb, Verworn, Davenport, Radl and Others 23 V Vi TABLE OF CONTENTS 2. More Thorough Experimental Analysis showing the Relative page Importance of Internal and External Factors in Behavior — Jennings, Holmes and Others 44 3. Summary of Historical Review 51 4. Various Definitions of Tropisms 53 5. Statement of Important Problems in the Study of Reactions to Light 57 PART II EXPERIMENTAL OBSERVATIONS AND DISCUSSIONS BEAR- ING ON THE QUESTION AS TO HOW ORGANISMS {ESPECIALLY THOSE WITHOUT EYES) BEND OR TURN AND MOVE TOWARD OR FROM A SOURCE OF STIMULATION CHAPTER IV Processes Involved in the Bending of Different Parts of Higher Plants toward the Source of Light I. Observations on Plumules of Indian Corn (Zea mays) and Leaves of Tropaeolum 59 a. Introduction; b. Apparatus; c. Experiments; d. Re- sults; e. Discussion. CHAPTER V Observations on Unicellular Forms in the Process of Attaining and Retaining a Definite Axial Position with Reference to the Source of Light 1. Myxomycetes and Rhizopods 74 2. Euglena 80 a. Description; b. Historical account; c. Orientation in light from two sources; d. Material; e. Method of loco- motion; /. Accuracy of orientation; g. Mechanics of orientation in Euglena x in the crawling state; h. Dis- cussion; i. Orientation of Euglena in the swimming state; j. Threshold or sensitiveness when different sur- faces are exposed to light; k. Function of the eye-spot. 3. Summary no TABLE OF CONTENTS Vll CHAPTER VI Observations on Unicellular Forms' in the Process of Attaining AND Retaining a Definite Axial Position with Reference to THE Source of Light (continued) PAGE 1. Stentor coeruleus 113 a. Introduction; b. Orienting reactions; c. Difference in sensitiveness with different surfaces illuminated; d. Lo- calized stimulation; e. Summary. 2. OeJjgonium Swarm-spores 123 a. Description; b. Material; c. Locomotion; d. Orienta- tion in Kght. 3. Trachelomonas 128 4. Chlamydomonas alboviridis (Stein) 131 5. Chlorogonium 134 6. Paramecium i34 CHAPTER VII The Factors Involved in the Process of Orientation in Colonial Forms 1. Volvox globator and minor 136 2. Pandorina and Eudorina 146 a. Function of the eye-spots. CHAPTER VIII Observations on the Responses Involved in the Regulation of Movement toward the Source of Light in Coelenterates 1. Hydra viridis 149 a. Historical review; b. Effect of light intensity on ac- tivity; c. Orientation and locomotion; d. Reactions of negative specimens; e. General conclusions. 2. Eudendrium Planulae 159 3. Eudendrium Hydranths 163 4. Reactions of Medusae 164 CHAPTER IX Regulation in the Direction of Movement with Reference to the Source of Light in Vermes, Fly Larvae, and Echinoderms I. Arenicola cristata — Larvae 166 a. Description; b. Locomotion; c. Orientation; d. Me- chanics of orientation; e. Discussion; /. Orienting stimulation; g. Summary. vm TABLE OF CONTENTS PAGE 2. Blowfly Larvae — Musca sp. (?) 175 a. Introduction; b. Locomotion; c. Accuracy of orienta- tion; d. Orientation in light from two sources; e. Orien- tation and movement — (ij perpendicular to the direc- tion of the rays — (2) toward a source of light; /. Sen- sitive region; g. Effect of light intensity on rate of locomotion; h. Method; i. Mechanics of orientation; j. Discussion; k. Summary. 3. Earthworms : L98 Summary 205 4. Planaria 206 Summary 210 5. Echinoderms 211 CHAPTER X Concerning the Question of Orientation in Mollusks, Arthropods AND Vertebrates, with Special Reference to Circus Movements • and their Bearing on this Question 1. General Account of Orientation 214 2. Circus Movements 215 3. Frogs and Toads 218 A. Bufo americanus. a. Method; h. Orientation in light from two sources; c. Orientation with one eye destroyed. 4. Caprella 223 a. Orientation; h. Discussion. 5. General Summary and Conclusions of Part II 228 PART III GENERAL CONSIDERATION OF REACTIONS TO LIGHT CHAPTER XI Adaptation, Formation of Aggregations in Regions of a Given Light Intensity and Different Methods of Response in Attaining this Region and Remaining in it. 1. Introduction sho\\ing that Reactions in general are Adaptive. 236 2. Different Reactions observed in the Process of Collecting in Regions having a given Condition of Illumination 239 TABLE OF CONTENTS ix a. Random movements and avoiding reactions; b. Orienta- page tion, change in sense of orientation, and avoiding re- actions; c. Orientation and extent of movement limited by environment; d. Orientation and movement directly toward the place where the organism comes to rest; e. Random movements and coming to rest in a given place. CHAPTER XII Reactions to Light which do not Result in Aggregation or Orientation 1. Reactions to Shadows — Protective 247 2. Reactions to Shadows — Procuring Food 249 3. Reactions to Sudden Increase of Light Intensity 250 4. Reactions to Light caused by the Effect of Continued Illumi- nation 252 5. Classification of Reactions to Light — Phototropism, Pho- topathy . 253 6. Reclassification of Reactions to Light 256 (i) On the basis of the character of the stimulus. a. Reactions to change of intensity; b. Reactions to con- stant illumination; c. Reactions of questionable cause. (2) On the basis of the fundamental causes of the response. a. Reactions caused by the direct effect of light on the reacting tissue; b. Reactions caused by an indirect effect of light; c. Reactions due, not to any effect of light in itself, but to what a given light condition or configura- tion may represent. 7. Evolution of the Reactions to Light 262 CHAPTER XIII Factors Involved in Regulating Reactions to Light — Variability and Modifi ability in Behavior I. Change in Sense of Reactions 265 a. Effect of intensity of light; b. Effect of change in tem- perature — Original observations; c. Effect of chemicals — Original observations; d. Effect of concentration of the medium and mechanical stimuli; e. Effect of internal changes. X TABLE OF CONTENTS CHAPTER XIV Factors Involved in Regulating Reactions to Light — Variability AND Modifiability IN Behavior (continued) PAGE 1. Changes in Sensitiveness, in the Optimum, and in Various Other Features regarding Reactions 288 2. General Summary of Part III 298 PART IV REACTIONS IN LIGHT OF DIFFERENT WAVE-LENGTHS OR ; COLORS CHAPTER XV Energy, Photochemical Reactions, and Brightness 1. Energy Distribution in the Spectrum 304 2. Brightness Distribution in the Spectrum 305 3. Distribution of Actinic Effect in the Spectrum 308 CHAPTER XVI Effect of Different Rays on the Reactions of Sessile Plants I. Summary 319 CHAPTER XVII The Relative Effect of Different Rays on the Reactions of Unicellular Forms 1. Strasburger's Experiments 321 2. Engelmann's Experiments 322 a. Diatoms and Oscillaria with different species of Navicula and Pinnularia as types; b. Cihates which have chlo- rophyll with Paramecium bursaria as a type; c. Flagel- lates with Euglena viridis as a type. 3. Verworn's Experiments 326 4. Experiments of Harrington and Leaming on Amoeba 327 5. Original Observations on Amoeba 328 a. Experiments with color filters; b. Experiments with the solar spectrum. TABLE OF CONTENTS XI CHAPTER XVIII Reactions of Multicellular Animals in Light Consisting of Waves Differing in Length PAGE 1. Experiments of Wilson on Hydra S33 2. Bert's Experiments on Daphnia 336 3. Lubbock's Experiments on Daphnia 337 4. Experiments of Yerkes on Simocephalus 341 5. Experiments of Graber on Various Animals 343 6. Loeb's Observations 346 CHAPTER XIX Brief Consideration of the Reactions of Multicellular Animals With Well-developed Eyes in Light Differing in Color — With Special Reference to Color Vision. 1. Ants 348 2. Bees 352 3. Higher Crustacea — Experiments of Minkiewicz 355 4. Fishes 358 5. General Summary and Conclusions of Part IV 360 CHAPTER XX Theoretic Considerations 366 Bibliography 379 Index 393 LIGHT AND THE BEHAVIOR OF ORGANISMS PART I INTRODUCTION AND HISTORICAL REVIEW CHAPTER I GENERAL INTRODUCTION That plants and animals respond to stimulation by light Is a matter of common Information. It Is also well known that many of the motile forms collect In regions of a given Intensity of light; that many orient, some moving or turning toward a source of light, others away from It; and that many go toward a source of light under certain conditions and away from It under others. The distribution of the power to respond to stimulation by light Ii> the plant and animal world has likewise been quite fully ascertained,^ and numerous accurate observations concerning the precise methods of response have been recorded. There is how- ever still much contention as to the explanation of these phenomena, and It Is this that concerns us chiefly In this work. In what manner and for what reasons do organisms collect In regions of certain light Intensity? How do they 1 See Wiesner, 1879, 1881; Verworn, i88g, pp. 35-61; Nagel, 1896; Davenport, 1897, pp. 182, 195; Radl, 1903, pp. 64-67; Washburn, 1908, pp. 120-147; Congdon, 1908. The works of these authors referred to by means of the dates following each name, as well as those of all other authors similarly referred to in the text, will be found in the bibliography. I 2 LIGHT AND THE BEHAVIOR OF ORGANISMS behave in light of different colors? What are the factors involved in orientation, i.e., in attaining a definite axial position with reference to the source of stimulation? How do organisms regulate the direction of movement; how do they remain oriented? What is the cause of reversal in the sense of orientation? What controls variability and modifiability in reactions to light? Are the reactions adaptive? These are the principal problems before us, problems which cannot fail to be of interest to all who are in any way concerned with the activities of organisms. Various solutions of these problems have been offered by different investigators. Some say that motile plants and animals orient and collect in light of a given intensity because the particular intensity in which they congregate pleases them more than any other, implying that there are psychic phenamena involved in the process and indicating that it is difference of light intensity in the field which controls the direction of movement. Others say the re- actions are not fundamentally adaptive and can be ex- plained mechanically; that the movements of organisms are, with few exceptions, regulated by the direction in which the rays of light penetrate the tissue or by the angle which the rays make with the sensitive surface or by the relative intensity on symmetrical opposite sides. Light is supposed by these investigators to act constantly as a directive stimulus. The organisms are automatically con- trolled by external factors. Still other authors claim that the reactions to light are in general useful to the organism, but that they can be accounted for mechanically and that the essential controlling factor is a change of intensity on the surface of the organism; that the other external factors mentioned are of Importance only in so far as they make such a change possible; that light does not act constantly as an orienting stimulus, and that internal physiological processes have much to do with the reactions. Some maintain that only the more refrangible rays of the spec- trum, those toward the violet end, are efficient in stimu- GENERAL INTRODUCTION 3 lating the organisms, others appear to be equally positive that all rays are active in this process, and still others say that the stimulating efficiency of different rays varies in different organisms and in the same organism under different conditions. Many investigators have apparently not thoroughly analyzed the problems concerning reactions: To them the question regarding orientation, e.g., has been merely: Is it ray-direction or intensity difference that regulates this? And with regard to this question they have failed to see that there may be a vast difference in effect between direction of rays in the field and direction through the organism; between diversity in light intensity in \he field and variation on different parts of the surface of the or- ganism. Moreover they have failed to appreciate the importance of difference in sensitiveness of different parts of the reacting organism, and the consequent effect of change in position on stirnulatlon. An illustration will serve to emphasize the Importance of distinguishing these characteristics. Suppose we have an elongated opaque organism the anterior end of which is more sensitive than the posterior, and suppose that this organism is in a field of direct sunlight without any other obstruction. Now It is evident that under such conditions the Intensity In the field is uniform, but the intensity on the Illuminated side of the organism may be almost infinitely higher than that on its shaded side, since no light can get through the organism, and If the organism changes Its axial relation with reference to the ray direction, the intensity on the surface may change just as much as it would if the organism moved about in a field in which the intensity was not uniform. Moreover if the organism takes a position in which the sensitive anterior end is shaded by the rest of the body it is of course in a lower effective inten- sity of light than it would be If this end were illuminated. Here again we see that a change in axial position In a field uniformly illuminated may produce the same effect as 4 LIGHT AND THE BEHAVIOR OF ORGANISMS movement from a region of one intensity to that of another. And all this is dependent upon the direction of the rays in the field whereas ray direction through the organism could have no such effect. It is evident then that the question " intensity difference or ray direction" may mean any one of several things. This loose way of stating the problems has led to much confusion. Let us then, first of all, attempt to get a clear under- standing of the questions involved in the reactions to light. With this in view we shall consider the origin and develop- ment of ideas concerning the movements in general of plants and animals, and those induced through stimulation by light in particular. CHAPTER II HISTORICAL REVIEW CONCERNING THE ORIGIN AND DE- VELOPMENT OF IDEAS AND THEORIES REGARDING MOVEMENTS IN PLANTS AND ANIMALS WITH SPECIAL REFERENCE TO THE QUESTION OF TROPISMSi I. Early Investigations and Ideas concerning Movement in Organisms To primitive man motion was the criterion of life. Everything that moved was aHve, not only plants and animals but also various elements in nature, — water, wind, fire, and the heavenly bodies. Motion was thought to be under the control of higher beings, or the result of the action of mind with which all living things were endowed. The philosophers of early civilized races abandoned the idea that all things which move are alive, but they still considered that all physiological processes are due to vital spirits. Aristotle (384-322 B.C.), thought that plants as well as animals had souls. The pith was supposed to be the seat of the soul in plants and all movements and other phenomena characteristic of living things were regarded as due to Its activity. During this period, all but a few thinkers seemed to rest content that nothing more could be learned about the cause or sequence of physiological processes, and these few made only feeble attempts from a ^ The following works are the main sources of information regarding the earlier views on plant and animal activity: History of Botany, by Julius von Sachs (1875), translation revised by I. B. Balfour, Oxford (1890); General Physiology, by Max Verworn (1894), translation second edition by F. S. Lee, New York (1899); Contemporary Psychology, by Guido Villa, translated by H. Manacorda, London, 1903. 5 6 LIGHT AND THE BEHAVIOR OF ORGANISMS philosophical point of view at further analysis of causa- tion. Not until the work of Galen (131-200 ± A.D.), four hundred years later, was there anything approaching ex- perimental analysis. 2. First Attempts at Mechanical Explanation of Life Phenomena Galen studied the structure of animals by direct obser- vation, even practicing vivisection on pigs and monkeys, and thus he sought to learn the functions of the various organs. But others did not continue the experimental work begun by him, and nearly thirteen centuries passed without any progress. It was not until early in the six- teenth century that interest in vital phenomena was again aroused, and it was a century later before Harvey made his important discovery on the circulatory system and pre- sented mechanical explanations for many factors involved in the process of circulation, all of them based on experi- mental evidence. A few years later, building on Descartes' idea " that the bodies of animals and men act wholly like machines and move in accordance with purely mechanical laws," Borelli undertook to reduce the movements of the organic motor apparatus to purely physical principles. The work of Borelli formed the foundation of the iatromechanical school, the members of which sought to explain all vital phenomena in animals by the application of physical prin- ciples. Other investigators of this period recognized the importance of chemical reactions in animal activity, and, under the leadership of Sylvius, founded the iatrochemical school, a school which admitted the importance of physical principles in explaining animal activity, but which strongly emphasized the influence of chemical phenomena in vital processes. The seventeenth century, and part of the eighteenth, formed a period in which mechanical explana- HISTORICAL REVIEW "J tlons were offered for practically all reactions and other physiological phenomena In animals, and the same may be said with regard to plants, as will be shown in the following pages. Toward the close of the seventeenth century, the striking movements of the sensitive plant, Mimosa, imported from America, attracted considerable attention. Ray described the movements of this plant In his " Historia Plantarum" (1693), and although an apparent believer in the soul of plants as defined by Aristotle, he tried to explain the move- ments mechanically. He thought that they were due not to sensations but to physical causes, — " Planta est corpus vivens non sentlens." The leaves remain erect, he said, because of the constant flow of sap Into them. When touched, the tubes which carry the sap to them are par- tially closed, and thus the supply of sap is diminished to such an extent that the leaves are no longer held erect and consequently droop. He was of the opinion, that plants bend toward a window because of difference In rate of growth on opposite sides due to difference In temperature. It was known in a general way that an increase in tempera- ture causes an Increase In the rate of growth In plants; and Sharroc had found that the stem on which he was experimenting grew toward that part of a window where air entered through an opening. It was from these obser- vations that Ray reached his conclusions. At about the same time Dodart came to the conclusion that physical contraction of the fibers on the moister side of roots and their expansion on the moister side of stems caused the former to turn down and the latter up. Du Hamel, after studying the effect of light, temperature and moisture on the direction of growth, concluded that the " Richtung der Dampfe " in the vessels and around the plant Is of prime Importance, and that if heat, light and moisture have any influence on the direction, It Is through their effect on the gases. Ridiculous explanations, all of them, in the light of present knowledge! But, even 8 LIGHT AND THE BEHAVIOR OF ORGANISMS SO, their importance cannot readily be overestimated, for they formed the foundation of later work which led to most fruitful results. 3. Period of Vitalism It was fully realized before the close of the eighteenth century that the mechanical explanations thus far pre- sented were inadequate to account for many fundamental phenomena at which they were directed. Especially was this true with reference to movements of various kinds, in both plants and animals. It led to the postulation of a controlling principle in living beings, foreign to chemistry and physics, a hypermechanical principle known as vital force. Those who believed in this principle were called vitalists. Some vitalists considered the postulated force inscrutable, and consequently abandoned all hope of gain- ing an insight into vital processes through experimental means. Others, however, among the foremost of whom were the botanist, De Candolle, and the famous physiologist, Johannes Miiller, held the opinion that this force was subject to further experimental analysis. The prevalence of the former view was however un- doubtedly the chief cause of stagnation in general physiology in its broadest sense, during this period, for there was no corresponding unproductive period in the development of physical sciences. As a matter of fact many who had been prominent investigators in both biological and physical sciences, now abandoned the former, and devoted their entire energies to the latter. 4. Return to Mechanical Explanations Miiller realized the weakness of the iatromechanical school as well as the inadequacy of pure philosophical speculation. On the one hand he recognized the importance HISTORICAL REVIEW 9 of speculation in guiding and unifying experimental work; on the other he saw the necessity of founding philosophical speculation on experimental facts. This broad view re- sulted in much comparative work especially in physiology and psychology, work which had a direct bearing on the nature of psychic processes as well as on the nature of physiological activity. Miiller worked on the higher animals almost exclusively. His aim was to analyze the phenomena of life as he found them in these organisms. His followers, Wohler, Liebig, Helmholtz, du Bois-Rey- mond, Lotze, Weber, Fechner and others, perpetuated this aim, but they did not retain his breadth of spirit. Some confined their investigations to the chemical side of physi- ology, others to the physical side, and still others to pure psychology. The question as to the origin and evolution of vital phenomena, especially psychic phenomena, was not yet prominent, if indeed it had been at all considered. The behavior of lower animals had been studied to some extent, but the Cartesian doctrine that there is no resemblance between the mind of man and that of animals was still very generally accepted. 5. Evolution and its Effect on the Study of Behavior of Plants and Animals With the establishment of the theory of evolution, there appeared a new incentive in the study of animal behavior. Darwin had demonstrated in a convincing manner the structural interrelationship between various animals, in- cluding man. It seemed clear that the complex anatomical structures found in the higher animals had their origin in the simpler structures found in the lower. Could the same be said with reference to behavior? Did the mental phenomena in man have their origin in the lower animals? If so, then there must be some evidence of mental activity in the lower animals, the psychic phenomena in these 10 LIGHT AND THE BEHAVIOR OF ORGANISMS organisms must resemble those in man, and the Cartesian doctrine must be wrong. The importance of this problem was at once recognized and a number of able investigators undertook its solution. Prominent among these may be mentioned Darwin, Paul Bert, Graber, Romanes, Lubbock and Preyer. It should be emphasized that these investigators were not primarily interested in explaining behavior either mechanically or otherwise. Their principal aim was to throw light on the origin of mental phenomena in man. Do the lower animals have sensations? Do they have memory? Do they rea- son? were questions which shaped their investigations. These questions they sought to answer by studying the behavior of animals under various conditions. Their results seemed to indicate that the psychic phenomena in animals differ from those in man in quantity rather than in quality. AVith reference to reactions to light they used what is known as the preference method. Experimental condi- tions were so arranged that the animals could get into light of different intensities or different colors. The kind of light in which they collected was supposed to be the kind they preferred. The work was weak in that only end results of the experiments were considered; it was never ascer- tained precisely how the animals got into the region in which they finally remained. Variation in the color or in the intensity of light in the field was to these investi- gators the controlling factor in the movement of animals. They failed to consider the possible effects of the direction of the rays, of variation in light intensity on the surface of the animals, and of various internal factors. This led to many erroneous conclusions. Still it must be said that whatever one may think as to the point of view of these investigators and the validity of their conclusions in general, one cannot read with unprejudiced mind the account of their work, especially that of Darwin, Lubbock, and Romanes, without greatly admiring the keenness of their HISTORICAL REVIEW II observations and the Ingenuity of their experiments. The point of view of these men dominated the field of animal behavior from the middle of the nineteenth century until the appearance of Verworn and Loeb well on toward 1890. As has been stated, they studied the behavior of animals with the express purpose of demonstrating the evolution of psychic phenomena in man. These investigators w^ere therefore not primarily interested in a physico-chemical explanation of animal behavior. The study of behavior in plants during this period was however pursued with a very different aim. The question as to the origin of mental phenomena influenced this study but little, for it was generally conceded that plants were devoid of all traces of psychic activity. There was con- sequently nothing left but to attempt to account for their behavior by means of physico-chemical analysis. Even the vitalists realized that In the attempt of such analysis lay the only hope of progress. 6. Introduction of the Term '' Tropism^' and Development of its Application to Different Reactions In 1806 De Candolle, a vltallst, succeeded in reversing the daily periodic sleep movements of leaves by exposing them to artificial light during the night and to darkness during the day. The same year Knight showed by at- taching developing seedlings to a rapidly revolving wheel that the direction of growth of roots and stems Is regu- lated by gravitation. He explained the directive action of gravitation by assuming "that the root, being of a semi- fluid consistence. Is bent downwards by its own weight, while the nutrient sap in the stem moves to the underside and causes stronger growth there, until by means of the curvature so produced the stem assumes the upright posi- tion." In 1828 Johnston found that roots In growing downward can overcome considerable resistance and that the direction of growth is therefore not due to their weight 12 LIGHT AND THE BEHAVIOR OF ORGANISMS as Knight had assumed. About the same time Dutrochet appHed endosmose and exosmose to explain the movement of plants mechanically. In 1833 De Candolle proved that it is light which causes plants to grow toward a window and not difference in temperature on opposite sides as Ray had thought one hundred forty years earlier. De Candolle discovered that light retards growth in plants and con- cluded that they turn toward a source of light because growth is retarded on the more highly illuminated side. The reaction according to De Candolle is due to difference in intensity of light on opposite sides. The turning toward the light was called heliotropism by De Candolle (1835, Vol. 2, p. 609), who was, according to Pfeffer (1906, pp. 154-155), the first to use this term. He used it merely to indicate the exciting agency and not to express the physiological response involved. Hofmeister (1863, p. 86) added the terms positive and negative heli- otropism; Frank (1870), invented the term geotropism; and Darwin (1881), Rothert (1896) and Massart (1902) intro- duced various special terms. While all these expressions were at first very generally used to designate the relation between the movement of the reacting organism and the source of stimulation, they soon came to be used to desig- nate also the processes underlying the reactions. De Candolle's explanation of the reaction to light assumed a direct effect of the external agent on the tissue involved in the reaction; and the same was true with reference to Knight's explanation of the reactions to gravity. The cells in which the processes producing the curvatures took place were supposed to be stimulated directly. The idea of irritability, of transmission of stimuli, of a differentiation between sensitive and reacting tissue, in plants had not yet been promulgated. The term " tropism" then gradually came to signify not merely turning, but turning due to the direct effect of the stimulating agent on the tissue produc- ing the movement, and this signification it has retained to some extent to the present time. HISTORICAL REVIEW 13 7. Further Analysis of Reactions in Plants to Light Sachs was the first to point out the inadequacy of the explanation brought forward by De Candolle. He and others found negative as well as positive plant structures in which the rate of growth was retarded by increase of inten- sity of light. The bending from the source of light in these structures could therefore not be due to difference in rate of growth on opposite sides induced by difference in illu- mination. Sachs was already of the opinion that gravita- tion does not control the direction of growth in plants by difference in the direct effect on the upper and lower sur- faces of the reacting organ as Knight had assumed. He says (1887, p. 696)/ " That in geotropic curvatures the important point is only as to the direction in which gravita- tion acts on the part of the plant, and that it is not in any way a matter of a stronger effect on the lower side and a feebler effect on the upper side, requires no proof." He was profoundly impressed by the similarity between the re- actions to light and those to gravity. This together with the inadequacy of the explanations of De Candolle and Knight led him to the conclusion clearly expressed in these words (1887, p. 695): " It necessarily followed from this that the standpoint assumed by De Candolle must be abandoned, and that the whole subject of heliotropism must be looked at in an entirely different way — a view which impressed me the more, since according to all the facts then known a striking agreement exists between heliotropic and geotropic effects, and at the same time I had even then come to see that geotropism and helio- tropism are to be looked upon as phenomena of irrita- bility. In addition to these reflections, also, I came to the conclusion that in heliotropic curvatures the important point is not at all that the one side of the part of the plant * The original German edition appeared in 1882. Sachs first announced his views on reactions to Hght in the preface of a paper by H. IMiiller in 1876. 14 LIGHT AND THE BEHAVIOR OF ORGANISMS is illuminated more strongly than the other, but that it is rather the direction in which the ray of light passes through the substance of the plant;" (1882, p. 851) . . . ''dass es sich bei den heliotropischen Kriimmungen gar nicht darum handle, dass die eine Seite des Pflanzentheils starker als die andere beleuchtet sei, dass es vielmehr nur auf die Richtung ankomme, in welcher der Lichtstrahl die Pflanzensubstanz durchsetzt." It will thus clearly be seen that the term " ray direction," so frequently used to characterize Sachs' view in opposition to intensity difference, is confusing. It expresses the truth, but not the whole truth. Sachs did not refer to ray direc- tion in general but to ray direction through the tissue, nor did he oppose intensity difference in general. He had nothing to do with the view of Bert and Graber that varia- tion in illumination of the field regulates reaction to light. He opposed the view of De CandoUe who states explicitly that it is difference of intensity on opposite sides of the reacting organ which causes heliotropic curvatures. In the study of the reactions of sessile plants to light there is but one phenomenon to consider — the turning of the plant or some of its parts so as to assume a definite position with reference to the source of light, i.e., orienta- tion. In motile forms we have not only to deal with the assumption of a definite axial position and movement but we have also to deal with the phenomenon of aggregation. How and why do certain unicellular organisms, for example, collect in dense masses in certain regions of their environ- ment? How is it that so many swarm spores, for instance, collect on the side of the dish toward the source of light? It was generally assumed that this phenomenon is due to difference of intensity in the field, that these organisms in some way select the illumination adapted to their needs and remain there. But Nageli had observed as early as i860 that flagellates and swarm spores collect at the side of a porcelain dish nearest the window although the inten- sity of light at this place is lower than elsewhere owing to HISTORICAL REVIEW I 5 the shadow produced by the side of the dish. This fact led some authors to conclude that these organisms avoid the light, but this did not account for the fact that the swarm spores collect also at the window side of a dish which pro- duces no shadow and in which this part is most highly illuminated. Cohn recognized this difficulty and con- cluded in 1865, eleven years before Sachs announced his ray-direction theory, that it is not difference of intensity in the field but direction of the rays that regulates the direction of movement in these organisms. He does not, however, make it clear whether he means direction of the rays through the tissue or direction in the field. Sachs answered the question as to the cause of aggre- gation in unicellular forms in a very simple way. He found (1876, p. 241) that certain inanimate particles suspended in water collect in definite regions when exposed to light owing to currents caused by variation in temperature. He was of the opinion that the movem.ent and aggregation of unicellular forms under similar conditions were largely if not entirely of the same nature. For the express purpose of testing this opinion, Stras- burger (1878, p. 552) studied the reactions of swarm spores to light. He repeated the experiments of Sachs and ob- tained confirmatory results, but concluded from detailed microscopic observations on the movements of these organ- isms that the aggregations formed in light under normal conditions are almost entirely due to active swimming of the swarm spores and not to currents in the water. Stras- burger in this paper, however, incidentally supports the general theory of Sachs on heliotropism. He found in agreement with Nageli's observation (i860) that positive swarm spores move toward a source of light even if in so doing they pass from regions of higher light intensity into regions of lower, and concluded just as Cohn (1865) had, that this cannot be due to difference of intensity. He does not however consider the fact that under the conditions of his experiments the anterior ends of the spores were con- 1 6 LIGHT AND THE BEHAVIOR OF ORGANISMS tinually more highly illuminated than the posterior, and that this difference of light intensity might determine the direction of movement; he merely states that this experi- ment shows that the reactions are due to ray direction without defining precisely what he means. Sachs, how- ever, in referring to these experiments says (1887, p. 696), " Even in the case of the influence of light on the move- ment of swarm spores, the important point can only be as to the direction of the rays of light, not as to whether the given swarm spore is illuminated more strongly in front or behind." The excellent observations of Engelmann (1882-1883) on the reactions of unicellular forms to light have a direct and important bearing on the question of aggregation. Stras- burger (1878) had observed that a sudden reduction of light causes a definite reaction in swarm spores — " zitternde Bewegung " — and others had seen similar responses to sudden changes in the intensity of other stimulating agents. But Engelmann seems to have been the first to point out clearly the relation between such responses and aggre- gation. He made detailed observations on the movements of Paramecium bursaria, Euglena viridis. Bacterium photo- metricum and other similar unicellular forms, in a field on a slide containing a spot more highly illuminated than the surrounding region. The illuminated spot, he says, acts like a trap; the organisms in their random movements swim into it without response, but when they reach the boundary on the way out, they stop suddenly, turn back, and thus remain in the illuminated area, which soon becomes crowded with them. These observations are of such vital impor- tance that it seems wise to emphasize them by quoting directly from the author. Regarding Paramecium bur- saria Engelmann says (1882, p. 393), " Ueberschreiten sie z.B. zufallig die Granze von Licht und Dunkel, oder tauchen sie auch nur mit der vorderen Halfte ihres Leibes eine Strecke weit in das Dunkel ein, so kehren sie sofort um nach dem Licht, wie wenn das Dunkel ihnen unangenehm HISTORICAL REVIEW 17 ware." Referring to the reaction of Euglena in a drop partially illuminated he says (1882, p. 395), " Dieses wirkt wie eine Falle, denn einmal hineingekommen, gehen die Euglenen in der Regel nicht wieder heraus. Sie kehren an der Grenze des Dunkels immer so gleich wieder urn ins Helle. Falls sie, was bei schnellem Vorwartsschwimmen wohl einmal geschieht, gans ins Dunkel hineingekommen sind, sistiren sie doch sofort die weitere Vorwartsbewegung, drehen um eine ihres kurzen Axen, probiren — oft unter bedeutenden Gestaltsanderungen — in verschiedenen Rich- tungen fortzukommen bis sie endlich wieder ins Licht gerathen." The effect of sudden reduction of light inten- sity on Bacterium photometricum is described in the follow- ing words (1883, p. no) : " Schwacht man nun plotzlich das Licht ... so sieht man alle bis dahin im Gesichtsfeld schwimmenden Bakterien fast im namlichen Moment eine Strecke weit zuruck schiessen, einige, meist unter leb- haftesten Rotation um ihre Langsaxe, stillstehen und danach wieder die gewohnliche Bewegung aufnehmen. Man erhalt vollstandig den Eindruck eines Erschreckens." According to Engelmann none of the organisms men- tioned above responds to an increase of intensity, nor do any of them respond to a decrease, if it is sufficiently gradual. The response is therefore dependent upon the time rate of change. Engelmann's account of aggregation in these organisms, as far as it goes, has stood the test of time. He failed however to grasp the importance of orientation and direct movement toward the optimum. The reactions to sudden changes of intensity described in this account are in all essentials like those discovered by Jennings some fifteen years later in his study of Paramecium. They have been designated Schreckbewegungen by Pfeffer and motor reflex and avoiding reaction by Jennings. They have much in common with the reactions to shadows in many higher forms, which Loeb (1893) claims are due to Unterschieds- empfindlichkeit and Bohn (1908) says are due to "sensibi- 1 8 LIGHT AND THE BEHAVIOR OF ORGANISMS lite differ entielle.'' The valuable experiments of Engelmann on the behavior of unicellular organisms in microspectra will be considered later (see Part IV). Several very important contributions to the knowledge of the reaction of plants, both in theory and in fact, were made by Charles Darwin and his son Francis, in their excellent work on " The Power of Movement in Plants " (1880). (i) They made detailed observations on the move- ment of different parts of plants in the absence of definite external stimulations, and found that practically all parts of plants — stems, leaves, roots, flowers, etc. — are constantly performing circumnutation movements. From this they concluded that tropic curvatures are brought about by modification of movements already present, i.e., that tropic stimuli are not the cause of movement but the cause of modification of movement. (2) They studied the reaction to light of plumules with the tips covered with small opaque caps; of radicles with the tips cauterized by the application of silver nitrate; and the reactions to gravity of radicles with the tips removed, and found that these structures responded normally after the tips were covered, removed or injured, provided that they had been previously stimu- lated, but that they did not respond if they were not stimu- lated until after the operation. From these results they concluded that plant-organs frequently have a sensitive part separated by some distance from a reacting part which is not sensitive, and that impulses originating in the former are transmitted to the latter. (3) They studied the reac- tions to light of certain plumules with one side covered with an opaque substance, and of others not covered but exposed at intervals, and concluded that the reactions are due to difference in intensity on opposite sides but that the principal factor in producing stimulation is a change of intensity rather than absolute difference of intensity. These conclusions are of such fundamental importance that it seems advisable to insert the following quotations from the authors' work cited above, (p. 485): "All ob- HISTORICAL REVIEW 1 9 servers apparently believe that light acts directly on the part which bends, but we have seen with the above described seedlings^ that this is not the case. Their lower halves were brightly illuminated for hours, and yet did not bend in the least towards the light, though this is the part which under ordinary circumstances bends the most." (p. 566), " We believe that this case [referring to an experiment of Wies- ner], as well as our own, may be explained by the excite- ment from light being due not so much to its actual amount, as to the difference in amount from that previously re- ceived; and in our case there were repeated alternations from complete darkness to light. In this, and in several of the above specified respects, light seems to act on the tissues of plants, almost in the same manner as it does on the nervous system of animals." (p. 567), " It is an inter- esting experiment to place caps over the tips of the cotyle- dons of Phalaris, and to allow a very little light to enter through minute orifices on one side of the caps, for the lower part of the cotyledons will then bend to this side, and not to the side which has been brightly illuminated during the whole time." (pp. 568-569), " In the case of the radicles of several, probably of all seedling plants, sensitiveness to gravitation is confined to the tip, which transmits an influ- ence to the adjoining upper part, causing it to bend towards the center of the earth. That there is transmission of this kind was proved in an interesting manner when horizon- tally extended radicles of the bean were exposed to the attraction of gravity for i or i\ h., and their tips were then amputated. Within this time no trace of curvature was exhibited, and the radicles were now placed pointing vertically downwards, but an influence had already been transmitted from the tip to the adjoining part, for it soon became bent to one side, in the same manner as would have occurred had the radicle remained horizontal and been still acted on by geotropism. Radicles thus treated con- tinued to grow out horizontally for two or three days, until ^ The tips of these were covered with opaque caps. 20 LIGHT AND THE BEHAVIOR OF ORGANISMS a new tip was reformed; and this was then acted on by geotropism, and the radicle became curved perpendicu- larly downwards." ^ (pp. 572-573), " We believe that there is no structure in plants more wonderful, as far as its functions are concerned, than the tip of the radicle. If the tip be lightly pressed or burnt or cut, it transmits an influence to the upper adjoining part causing it to bend away from the affected side; and, what is more surprising, the tip can distinguish between a slightly harder and softer object, by which it is simultaneously pressed on opposite sides. If, however, the radicle is pressed by a similar object a little above the tip, the pressed part does not transmit any influence to the more distant parts, but bends abruptly towards the object. If the tip perceives the air to be moister on one side than on the other, it likewise transmits an influence to the upper adjoining part, which bends towards the source of moisture. When the tip is excited by light (though in the case of radicles this was ascertained in only a single instance), the adjoining part bends from the light; but when excited by gravitation the same part bends towards the center of gravity. In almost every case we can clearly perceive the final purpose or advantage of the several movements. Two, or perhaps more, of the exciting causes often act simultaneously on the tip, and one conquers the other, no doubt in accordance with its importance for the life of the plant. The course pursued by the radicle in penetrating the ground must be determined by the tip; hence it has acquired such diverse kinds of sensitiveness. It is hardly an exaggeration to say that the tip of the radicle thus endowed, and having the power of directing the movements of the adjoining parts, acts like the brain of one of the lower animals; the brain being seated within the anterior end of the body, receiving ^ This experiment was first performed by Ciesielski (1875). Darwin's interpretation of the results has been questioned. See Francis Darwin's interesting presentation of the controversy concerning this and related sub- jects (1907, PP- 35-42; 69-76). HISTORICAL REVIEW 21 Impressions from the sense-organs, and directing the several movements." This work of Darwin seems to have been set aside by some of the most prominent Investigators of the day and has even to this time not received recognition In accord with Its importance. Loeb does not mention It at all. Sachs refers to It In the following terms (1887, p. 689): " In such experiments with roots not only Is great precau- tion necessary, but also the experience of years and an extensive knowledge of vegetable physiology, to avoid falling Into errors, as did Charles Darwin and his son Francis, who, on the basis of experiments which were unskilfully made and Improperly explained, came to the conclusion, as wonderful as it was sensational, that the growing-point of the root, like the brain of an animal, dominates the various movements in the root." The very point which Sachs rejects has however been confirmed by Pfeffer (1894), Czapek (1895, p. 244), Rothert (1894, p. 3), and others. Czapek's experiment bearing on this point is ingenious and convincing. He forced the apex of radicles n Fig. I. I. Seedlings of Lupinus albus (smaller size). The seedling (A) has been removed from the klinostat after the apex is fixed in the glass cap k, and after twenty-four hours has curved so as to place itself parallel with the perpendicular line shown by the arrow. After Czapek, from Pfeffer (1906). II. Seedlings of Setaria italica. The roots have been cut away down to the rudiments w, the cotyledon [plumule] fixed in the glass tube a, and the seedling is then placed horizontally. In A the hypocotyl has curved through 180°, and at B has formed a complete coil. (Twice enlarged). After Darwin, from Pfeffer (1906). while being rotated on a clinostat to grow into small bent tubes of glass closed at one end. When the seedlings were 2 2 LIGHT AND THE BEHAVIOR OF ORGANISMS permanently fastened so that the base of the radicle was horizontal and the tip vertical, there was no reaction, but when so fastened that the base was vertical and the tip horizontal, they responded by bending in the region above the glass tube until the tip became vertical. (See Fig. I.) CHAPTER III HISTORICAL REVIEW CONCERNING THE ORIGIN AND DEVEL- OPMENT OF IDEAS AND THEORIES REGARDING MOVE- MENTS IN PLANTS AND ANIMALS WITH SPECIAL REFERENCE TO THE QUESTION OF TROPISMS (continued) I. The Application of tJie Underlying Principle of Tropisms in the Study of Animal Behavior as opposed to this Study from the Point of View of Comparative Psychology Seven years after the appearance of " The Power of Movement In Plants," by Darwin, Loeb began his work on behavior of animals, at Wiirzburg, in an atmosphere per- vaded by the spirit of Sachs. His first paper on the subject, entitled " Die Orientierung der Thiere gegen das Licht (thierischer Heliotropismus)," appeared in January, 1888. A far more important and extensive paper bearing the title " Der Heliotropismus der Thiere und seine Ueberein- stimmung mit dem Heliotropismus der Pflanzen," was brought out in pamphlet form the following year. Other shorter papers followed from time to time. Most of these papers, originally published in German, were translated and published in English in Loeb's " Studies in General Physiology," Chicago, 1905. These translations will be referred to almost exclusively in the following pages. Loeb took up the work in animal reactions with the idea of explaining such reactions on chemical and physical bases in opposition to the so-called anthropomorphic explanations current at the time. His object was " to find the agencies which determine unequivocally the direc- tion of motion in animals." He writes (1905, Preface), " I consider a complete knowledge and control of these agencies the biological solution of the metaphysical problem 23 24 LIGHT AND THE BEHAVIOR OF ORGANISMS of animal instinct and will." The author assumed that these agencies had been fairly definitely ascertained with reference to plants, and it was generally conceded that their movements were not influenced by psychic phenomena. He therefore began his work by attempting to show that the reactions in plants and animals are controlled by the same agencies, with the express purpose of proving that the reactions of animals are not due to subjective (anthropo- morphic) sensations as the work of Bert, Graber, Lubbock, Romanes and others might lead one to infer. " I consider it inadvisable," he says (1905, p. 16), ''to represent the movements observed in animals as the expression of a 'color preference', or a 'color sensation', of a 'pleasurable' or 'unpleasurable sensation', as do most animal physiolo- gists and zoologists who have studied the effects of light in the animal kingdom." (1906, p. 125), " It seemed to me that we had no right to see in this tendency of animals to fly into flame the expression of an emotion, but that this might be a purely mechanical or compulsory effect of the light, identical with the heliotropic curvature observed in plants. I believed that the essential effect of the light upon these animals might consist in a compulsory automatic turning of the head toward the source of light, corresponding to the turning of the head, or the tip, of a plant stem toward the light; and that the process of moving toward the source of light was only a secondary phenomenon. It seemed to me also that if the stem of the plant could suddenly acquire the power of locomotion, it would act exactly like the animals which fly into the flame." In his first paper Loeb deals with the reactions of certain insect larvae. He found that positive larvae go toward the light even when conditions are so arranged that in so doing they must go into light of lower intensity. These results lead to the following conclusions (1888, p. 2): " Die Orientirung der Thiere gegen eine Lichtquelle wird bei den Pflanzen (J. v. Sachs) bedingt durch die Richtung, in welcher die Lichtstrahlen die thierischen Gewebe durchset- HISTORICAL REVIEW 25 zen, und nicht durch die Unterschiede in der Lichtinten- sitat auf den verschiedenen Seiten des Thieres." It is evident from this quotation that Loeb at this time held that the direction of the rays through the tissue is the con- trolHng factor in orientation of animals; that is, that orienta- tion in animals takes place just as Sachs had said it does in plants; that it is not due to difference of intensity on different parts of the organism, but to the direction in which the directive rays pass through the tissue. The results recorded in the second paper, dated 1889, are in all essentials like those found in the first. The principal points established are (i) that positive animals will pro- ceed toward the window under conditions such that they continually get into weaker light; (2) that only the more refrangible rays are active in causing reactions. From these results Loeb concludes as follows (1905, p. 3; first edition, 1889): " The conditions which control the movements of animals toward light are identical, point for point, with those which have been shown to be of paramount influence in plants.'' Five conditions are considered: (i) ray direction; (2) wave length; (3) constancy of intensity; (4) limits of intensity; (5) temperature. Two of them, the first and the third, are of special interest to *is at present. I shall therefore quote Loeb's words with reference to them* (1905, p. 2), "So far as the light is concerned, the circumstance which controls the orientation of the animal and the direc- tion of its movements is the direction of the rays falling upon the animal. The condition which is of importance on the part of the animal is the symmetrical shape of the body." It consequently appears that he, at this time, no longer considered the direction in which the rays pass through the tissue of the organism of special importance but that he still regarded the direction in which they fall upon it of importance. At the same time, however, he accepted Sachs' theory as giving an adequate explanation of orientation in plants and claimed that this theory also holds for animals, for he says (1905, p. 89), "I showed 26 LIGHT AND THE BEHAVIOR OF ORGANISMS that the law put forward by Sachs for the heliotropism of plants, namely, that the direction of the rays of light determines the orientation, holds good also for animals." Elsewhere in the same paper he states this law explicitly as follows (1905, p. 5): "Sachs came to the conclusion that the direction in which the rays of light penetrate the plant tissue determines the orientation of the plant toward light." This statement of the law is correct, but it should be emphasized that Sachs also said " that in heliotropic curvatures the important point is not at all that the one side of the part of the plant is illuminated more strongly than the other." There is evidently much confusion here in the application of Sachs' theory. Do Loeb's conclusions in this paper show " that the law put forward by Sachs for heliotropism of plants . . . holds good also for animals"? He writes (1905, p. 28): " From what has been said, no one, I believe, will doubt that the direction of the progressive movements of the caterpillars of Porthesia chrysorrhoea is determined by the direction of the rays of light, and not by differences in the intensity of the light in different parts of space. Posi- tively heliotropic animals are compelled to turn their oral pole toward the source*of light and to move in the direction of the rays toward this source." And (1905, p. 53), " The direction of the rays, and not the distribution of the intensity of the light, in the test-tube, therefore, determines the direction of the progressive movements.''' From these quotations it is evident that Loeb means ray direction in general in opposi- tion to difference in intensity in the field. He proved that under the conditions of his experiments the direction of motion is not governed by the difference of intensity in the field. But this has nothing to do with Sachs' theory, for this theory does not consider the effect of ray direction in the field or " distribution of the intensity in the test- tube." Sachs, as stated above, says very definitely that it is the direction in which the rays pass through the tissue and not difference of light intensity on opposite sides of the HISTORICAL REVIEW 27 organism which regulates the movement. Consequently if Loeb's explanation holds for animals and Sachs' for plants, it is clear that the orientation in animals is not necessarily regulated in the same way as in plants. Sachs opposed the idea of De Candolle that difference of intensity on opposite sides of the reacting organism con- trols orienting reactions; while Loeb at this time opposed the idea of Bert and Graber that difference of intensity in the field determines the place of aggregation, and that animals are " unterschiedsempfindlich." Sachs argued in favor of ray direction through the tissue of the reacting organ, Loeb in favor of ray direction in general. Failure to recognize the difference between these views has led to much confusion. It is on this account that the problem has generally been so loosely stated in the terms ''Is it ray direction or intensity difference? " — a question which evidently cannot be answ^ered without an explicit state- ment of the sense in which these terms are used. Do Loeb's experimental results prove the absence of sensations as factors in animal behavior as he assumes? The experiments on which he bases his conclusions are similar to those of Strasburger on swarm spores referred to on p. 15. Loeb found that positive animals very gen- erally move toward a source of light even if in so doing they pass into regions of lower light intensity. He con- cluded from this result correctly that this cannot be due to variation in the intensity of light in the space, but incorrectly that this disproves the existence of sensation, for the animals with which he worked are more sensitive to light at the anterior than at the posterior end. If they enjoy light one would expect them to continue to face its source even if the general illumination is decreased, be- cause, if they should turn, the sensitive anterior end would become shaded and this would cause a decrease in the pleasant effect of light. The experimental results just cited, therefore, do not prove the absence of sensation as a controlling factor in the behavior of animals; neither do 2 8 LIGHT AND THE BEHAVIOR OF ORGANISMS they show that It is not difference in light intensity on the surface of the reacting organism which regulates orientation. Let it be clearly understood that I am not arguing in favor of psychic phenomena as factors in orientation. Loeb's greatest service to the study of animal behavior was his strenuous opposition to this idea, in spite of his failure to demonstrate the absence of sensation as a factor in reactions. Let us now turn more directly to Loeb's later views on orientation, or tropisms. These are clearly expressed and explicitly stated in the following quotations. Referring to the analogy between the effect of a constant electric current and light, Loeb says (1897, p. 440): " Wir linden hier erstens Wirkungen, die bei constanter Intensitat des Lichtes unverandert andauern. Das sind die helio- tropischen Wirkungen, die auf dem Einfluss des Lichtes auf die Spannung assoziirter Muskelgruppen beruhen ('das Licht wirkt bei constanter Intensitat dauernd als heliotropische Reizursache auf die Thiere') . . . Ich glaube jetzt, dass hier eine vollkommene Analogie der Licht- und Stromwirkungen zu Tage tritt, derart, dass auch, wie beim Strom, die Licht-intensitdt dauernd die Spannung der Muskeln beeinflusst, dass aber die Steilheit der Intensi- tdtsschwankung die Fortleitung der Spannungsdndericng bestimmt. " Aber die Analogie zwischen der Stromwirkung und der Lichtwirkung geht weiter. Als den fiir die heliotropische Orientirung der Thiere wesentlichen ausseren Umstand wies ich die Richttmg der Lichtstrahlen nach, wie das Sachs bereits friiher fiir die Pflanzen gethan hatte. Das Wesen der Orientirung fasste ich dahin auf, dass bei vollendeter Orientirung Symmetriepunkte der Oberfldche des Thieres unter gleichem Winkel von den Lichtstrahlen getroffen werden.'' An explanatory footnote (1905, p. 2), dated 1903, reads as follows: ■" In these experiments it is presumed that the animals move under the influence of only one source of light. It is explicitly stated in this and the following papers HISTORICAL REVIEW 29 that If there are several sources of Hght of unequal inten- sity, the Hght with the strongest intensity determines the orientation and direction of motion of the animal. Other possible complications are covered by the unequivocal statement, made and emphasized in this and the following papers on the same subject, that the main feature in all phenomena of heliotropism is the fact that symmetrical points of the photosensitive surface of the animal must be struck by the rays of light at the same angle. It is in full harmony with this fact that if two sources of light of equal intensity and distance act simultaneously upon a helio- tropic animal, the animal puts its median plane at right angles to the line connecting the two sources of light. This fact was not only known to me, but had been demon- strated by me on the larvae of flies as early as 1887, in Wiirzburg, and often enough since. These facts seem to have escaped several of my critics." In these papers it is clear that the important factors in orientation to light are considered to be: (i) symmetry of the body; (2) the angle between the rays and the sensitive surface on opposite sides; and (3) constant intensity, functioning as it does in case of the electric current. Orien- tation in light is supposed to be controlled by the direct action of the external agent, on the locomotor tissue or through a direct reflex arc. It is controlled unequivocally by the external agent, which acts constantly as a directive stimulus similar to the action of a constant electric current. At this time Loeb evidently still placed much dependence upon the assumed effect of the angle which the rays make with the sensitive surface (ray direction), for if he con- sidered merely intensity difference on opposite sides it would be impossible for him to say as he does that when organisms are exposed to light from " several sources . . . of unequal intensity, the light with the strongest intensity determines the orientation and direction of motion of the animal." In a more recent discussion however he uses the following expression (1906, p. 130)1 " We started w^th 30 LIGHT AND THE BEHAVIOR OF ORGANISMS the assumption that the hehotropic reactions are caused by a chemical effect of light; in all such reactions time plays a role. We assume, furthermore, that if light strikes the two sides of a symmetrical organism with unequal inten- sity, the velocity or the character of the chemical reactions in the photosensitive elements of both sides of the body is different." This and the following quotation show that he now considers orientation to be controlled by difference of intensity on opposite sides, the very idea which Sachs in his theory opposed. In the following quotation he also brings out his idea as to the direct effect of the agent on the reacting tissue with reference to plants. Orientation in animals is supposed to be just like this in principle; in animals, the agent is sup- posed to affect the locomotor organs directly or through a direct reflex arc (1906, p. 118) : '' How can light bring about heliotropic curvatures? Let us suppose that light strikes a plant on one side only, or more strongly on one side than on the opposite side, and that it be absorbed in the super- ficial layers of tissue of that side. In this case we assume that on that side certain chemical reactions occur with greater velocity than on the opposite side. What these reactions are is unknown; we may think provisionally of oxidations. This change in the velocity of chemical re- actions either produces a tendency of the soft elements on that side to contract a little more than on the opposite side, or creates otherwise a greater resistance to those forces which have a tendency to elongate or stretch the plant, e.g., hydrostatic pressure inside the cells, or imbibi- tion of certain tissue elements. The outcome will be that one side of the stem will be stretched more than the oppo- site side, and this will bring about a curvature of the stem. Where the latter is soft at the tip, the bending will occur only, or chiefly, in that region; and as the degree of softness decreases rapidly from the tip downward, the result will be that the tip will bend toward the source of light. This result may possibly be aided by a greater photosensitive- HISTORICAL REVIEW 3 1 ness of the extreme tip of the stem, although I am not aware that this is an established fact." It is strange that such a theory should have been sug- gested to explain heliotropic curvatures in plants twenty- six years after Darwin (see p. 18) proved that only the tips of certain radicles and plumules are sensitive to light and that the region where the curvature takes place is fre- quently not at all sensitive, and several years after Pollock (1900) had shown that traumatic stimuli are in many instances transmitted from the tip of radicles to the motory zone 5 to 8 mm. distant and produce curvatures toward the uninjured side even if the cortex, the conducting tissue, is cut on the side between the point of stimulation and the motory zone. Moreover Loeb's theory fails utterly to account for curvatures in structures having but a single cell cavity as, for example, Vaucheria, the rhizoids of liver- worts, and the hyphae of molds, all of which were known to respond to light long before his theory was formulated. Loeb's idea that the movements in plants and anim.als are unequivocally controlled by external agents is emphasized in the following quotations: (1905, p. 107), "By the help of these causes it is possible to control the ' voluntary ' movements of a living animal just as securely and une- quivocally as the engineer has been able to control the movements in inanimate nature"; (1906, p. 128), "It should be observed that the essential feature in these re- actions is the compulsory turning of the head by the light, which leaves the animal no choice, making all the cater- pillars of Porthesia or all the plant lice of the same culture behave exactly alike, just as in the case of a magnet all the pieces of iron are compelled to behave ahke"; (1906, p. 124), " The light would turn them automatically until their axis of symmetry was in the direction of the rays of light, and theanimal could then move only in this direction." Thus we have seen that in 1906 Loeb asserts that orien- tation in light is unequivocally controlled by the relative intensity on symmetrically located sensitive parts of the 32 LIGHT AND THE BEHAVIOR OF ORGANISMS organism; that light stimulates the locomotor organs con- tinuously and directly or through a direct reflex arc. When both sides are not equally illuminated one moves faster than the other, causing the organism to turn until the light intensity on the two sides is equal when they are both equally stimulated and consequently move at the same rate. This view he apparently still holds for he affirms it in unquestionable terms in a recent address (1909, pp. 9-15): " Zwei Faktoren bestimmen die Progressivbewegung der Tiere unter diesen Bedingungen; der eine ist die symme- trische Strukturdes Tieres und der zweite die photochemische Wirkung des Lichtes (p. 9). . . . Wenn nun mehr Licht auf eine Retina fallt als auf die andere, so werden auch die chemischen Reaktionen, Beispielsweise die organischen Oxydationen, in einer Retina mehr beschleunigt als in der •andern; und dementsprechend werden in dem einen op- tischen Nerven starkere chemische Anderungen auftreten als in dem anderen (p. 11). . . . Diese Ungleichheit der che- mischen Prozesse pflanzt sich von den sensiblen in die motorischen Nerven und schliesslich in die mit denselben verbundenen Muskeln fort. Wir schliessen daraus, dass bei gleicher Beleuchtung der beiden Retinae die symmetrische Muskelgruppe beider Korperhalften in gleicher Weiser chemisch beeinflusst werden und somit in den gleichen Kontractionszustand geraten ; wahrend wenn die Reaktions- geschwindigkeit ungleich ist, die symmetrischen Muskeln auf einer Seite des Korpers in starkere Tatigkeit geraten, als auf der andern Seite. Das Resultat einer solchen un- gleichen Tatigkeit der symmetrischen Muskeln beider Korperhalften ist eine Anderung der Bewegungsrichtung des Tieres " (p. 12). In his earlier work Loeb appears to have held that all reactions to light are due to constant intensity, but later (1893, p. 265) he recognizes that some are due to change in intensity. The former he calls heliotropic, the latter photokinetic {unter s chieds empfindlich) . He characterizes the difference between the two thus (1906, p. 135): " Helio- HISTORICAL REVIEW 33 tropism covers only those cases where the turning to the Hght is compulsory and irresistible, and is brought about automatically or mechanically by the light itself. On the other hand, there are compulsory and mechanical reactions to light which are not cases of heliotropism; namely, the reaction to sudden changes in the intensity of light." Orien- tation is therefore, according to Loeb, never due to change in light intensity. ''At a constant intensity light acts as a continuous source of stimulation." When animals are not oriented both sides are continuously stimulated but one is stimulated more than the other. This causes one side to move faster than the other " until symmetrically situated points on the body of the animal are struck at the same angle by equally strong rays of light." In a recent paper (1907) Loeb again emphasizes this difference between " heliotropism " and '' Vnterschieds- empfindlichkeit.'' It is therefore evident that he was well aware of the fact that certain animals respond to changes in light intensity. This, however, is an old idea. As a matter of fact it was the fundamental postulate of all who thought that reactions are controlled by psychic phenomena. And in his earlier work Loeb attempted to prove the ab- sence of such phenomena, by showing that aggregation of animals in a given light intensity is not due to difference of intensity, i.e., that the animals are not "unterschiedsemp- findlich.'' Later, however, he found that planarians collect in regions of lowest intensity because they are " unter- schiedsempfindlich "; (1907), '' Both forms of reaction may occur in the same animal (e.g., Spirographis) , but this is neither necessary nor the rule." Loeb did not study the reactions of unicellular organisms to light and it has been frequently stated that he did not apply his theory to their reactions. Such statements, how- ever, are erroneous as the following quotations will show: (1905, p. 73), " Experiments on infusoria are already suffi- ciently complete to show that Sachs's laws of heliotropism also hold good for them. . . . Trembley's experiments on 34 LIGHT AND THE BEHAVIOR OF ORGANISMS Hydra, however, show that in their case also the relation is the same; at least it seems to me that Trembley's experi- ments cannot be interpreted unless we assume that the progressive movements of Hydra are determined by the direction of the rays of light." I have quoted Loeb rather freely in trying to present his views, mainly because he and others have repeatedly main- tained that critics have failed to understand his work, particularly that referring to the cause of orientation and aggregation in regions of certain intensity. These quota- tions together with the discussion presented seem to warrant the following summary statements concerning his work on reactions to light. (i) His object was to give a mechanical explanation of behavior in opposition to so-called anthropomorphic ex- planations of Bert, Graber and others. (2) He proposed to do this by showing that the reactions in animals, especially those due to stimulation by light, are governed by the same law as those in plants. (3) He accepted the explanation of orientation in plants given by Sachs and states his theory correctly. Loeb's conclusions however do not support this theory. He confuses ray direction through the tissue with ray direction in the field and difference of intensity on the surface of the organism with diversity of intensity in the field. (4) Loeb failed to consider the effect of difference in sensitiveness to light between the posterior and anterior ends of animals and the effect of change in axial position on the relative illumination of these ends. (5) His experimental evidence does not prove that the direction of light rays functions in orientation except in so far as it may produce difference of intensity on the surface of the organism; nor does it prove the absence of sensation in orientation. (6) In 1888 Loeb held that orientation in animals is controlled by the direction in which the rays of light pass through the tissue. From 1889 to 1903 he advocated the HISTORICAL REVIEW 35 idea that orientation is controlled by the direction in which the rays strike the surface, or the angle they make with the surface. His statements from 1906 to 1909 indicate that he thinks that orientation is regulated by the relative inten- sity of light on symmetrically located sensitive structures on opposite sides of the organism, a view which Sachs strenuously opposed. (7) Loeb's theory of orientation with reference to plants implies that the external agent acts on the motor apparatus directly, and with reference to animals that it acts either on the motor apparatus directly or through a direct reflex arc. (8) He thinks that movements in plants and animals are controlled unequivocally by external agents and that they are not fundamentally adaptive. " Eine 'Auswahl' einer passenden Beleuchtungsintensitat habe ich nie beobachtet" (1909, p. 35). (9) Reactions to light may be heliotropic or photokinetic. The former are never due to change in light intensity, they " are a function of the constant intensity; (the latter) a function of the quotient of the change of intensity over time," i.e., rate of change of intensity. There is a perfect analogy between the effect of light and the effect of a constant electric current. (10) Aggregation in some forms is due to photokinetic reactions. (11) Loeb considers his theory applicable to the reactions of the infusoria as well as to those of higher animals and plants. (12) He stands for an objective explanation of the be- havior of animals in all his work, but he cannot be con- sidered as the originator of this idea. Nor was he the first to attempt to put it on an experimental basis. Verworn was one of the first investigators in comparative physiology in its broadest sense. He was of the opinion that the fundamental physiological and psychological pro- cesses are common to all animals and that they can be solved in the simple forms more readily than in the more 36 LIGHT AXD THE BEHAVIOR OF ORGANISMS complex. In this connection we are interested primarily only in his investigations on the activities of the protozoa. These were taken up in 1886, two years before Loeb's first preliminary note on the reactions of animals appeared. Verworn was probably the first to attempt an explanation of the behavior of animals from a purely objective point of view. In his papers many valuable observations are re- corded on the collection of protozoa in given regions, and on the orientation of these creatures when subjected to stimuli of various sorts. Contrary to the idea of Loeb, he concluded that the reactions to light are fundamentally adaptive (1899, p. 60). His explanation of orientation is of particular interest to us since it has frequently been referred to in works on behavior. This he has presented in his General Physiology^ (1899, P- 499) : "We will examine, first, the forms that possess one fiagellum, such as many Bacteria and flagellate I?ifusoria, and will select as repre- sentative the delicate, green, flagellate- infusorian Euglena, which, in summer, ^-j".^ ^.''5 by means of its countless numbers, ^^ changes the water of standing pools into a deep green. The fiagellum of the Flagellata is upon the anterior pole of the body and moves through the water in a screw-like path. For the sake of simplicity its motion may be considered as taking place in a single Fig. 2. Scheme of the j^j^g_ j^ j^ ^^^^ ^^^^ ^^^^ -^ ^^^ij, contraction of the ilagel- ^ lum of a flageilate-infus- lates about the Straight middle position '^i^t^"s:nZ [Fig- 2I by means of alternate rhythmic contractions toward the right (b) and toward the left (bi) ; the swing out of the middle posi- tion (a) into one of the two extreme positions (b or bi) represents the phase of contraction; the return from one 1 The first edition of this volume appeared in 1894 at a time when Loeb was emphasizing the importance of ray direction more strongly than he did later. HISTORICAL REVIEW 37 of the extreme positions into the middle position, the phase of expansion. The fiagellum works, therefore, Hke an oar that is moved alternately to the right and to the left at the bow of a boat. It is evident that, while undisturbed and having equal conditions upon all sides, the infusorian body must move forward in a straight line, if the fiagellum beats equally strongly toward the right and toward the left, i.e., if contraction and expansion occur with equal rapidity toward the two sides. But if a contractile stimulus acts upon the flagellate suddenly from one side, and if the long axis of the body is not already turned in the direction of the stimulus with the posterior pole toward its source, such a position is assumed by means of a few strokes of the fiagellum; for with every oblique or transverse position of the long axis the fiagellum is stimulated to contract more strongly upon the side upon which the stimulus falls than upon the opposite side, it makes stronger strokes toward the former than toward the latter side, and the result is that the anterior part of the body is turned away from the source of the stimulus. Exactly the same relations exist here as in a boat moved by a single oar. The bow of the boat also turns toward the opposite side when the boat is propelled more strongly upon one side than the other. The unequal strength of the flagellar stroke in the two directions continues, and the anterior part of the body is turned constantly more away from the source of the stimulus, until the body has placed its long axis in the direction of the incident stimulus. Then both sides of the fiagellum become equally stimulated and the protist swims in a straight line, so long as the stimulus continues. Thus, negative chemotaxis, phototaxis, etc., appear in uniflagellated Bacteria and Flagellata as a necessary result of a unilateral excitation of contraction in the flagellum." Orientation in forms possessing two fiagella and in forms possessing numerous cilia is similarly explained. When an organism of this sort is not oriented it is assumed that the flagella or the cilia are more strongly stimulated on one side 38 LIGHT AND THE BEHAVIOR OF ORGANISMS than on the other and that this causes them to beat more or less effectively until the organism becomes directed toward or from the source of stimulation, a direction it must retain. By careful reading of Verworn's theory, quoted above, one is led to infer that he considered the flagella or cilia to be stimulated directly. This, however, is not an essential part of the theory. The essential point is that there is a difference in the effect of the beat of the cilia on opposite sides when these sides are differently illuminated. It does not matter whether this is caused directly by the effect of the stimulating agent on the cilia or indirectly through impulses transmitted to the cilia from the body protoplasm. An organism once oriented in accord with this theory must remain oriented unless it is thrown out of orientation by some other agent than that which has caused the orienta- tion. Orientation according to this theory is direct. Light acts constantly as a directive stimulus. Difference of in- tensity on opposite sides of the organism causes unequal action of the cilia on the two sides. Symmetrical location of organs is essential in the organism. It will thus be seen that Verworn's theory of tropisms agrees with the theories of Loeb, especially the more recent, in all essential points. These two authors, however, opposed each other from the beginning. Loeb argued in favor of ray direction, Verworn in favor of intensity difference; neither seems to have known precisely what the other meant. Verworn gives the following statement (1899, p. 450) : " From the preceding consideration and by analogy with the directive effects of other stimuli it is evident that only the difference in the intensity of the light upon differ- ent parts of the body can produce a directive effect; where the stimulus acts upon the surface of the body from all sides with equal intensity, the reason for a definite axial position disappears, as is to be observed most clearly in the action of chemical stimuli upon all sides. Although this is obvious, some investigators, such as Sachs and Loeb, have HISTORICAL REVIEW 39 believed that the direction of the rays is more responsible for the manifestation of phototactic phenomena than are differences in intensity. It is difficult to conceive this, for, since the assumption of an axial direction is possible only when differences exist at two different points of the surface of the body, it is wholly mystical how the direction of the rays, which is the same upon all sides of the body, can pro- duce such an effect." Loeb is here classified with Sachs where he claimed to belong. His experimental results and conclusions are, however, from the beginning, more nearly in harmony with the theory of Verworn than they are with that of Sachs. Verworn considers his theory applicable to orienting reactions in unicellular forms induced by stimuli of various kinds. He says (1899, P- 5^3) > " Thus the phenomena of positive and negative chemotaxis, barotaxis, thermotaxis, phototaxis and galvanotaxis which are so highly interesting and important in all organic life, follow with mechanical necessity as the simple results of differences in biotonus, which are produced by the action of stimuli at two different poles of the free-living cell." In 1892 Oltmanns attempted to settle the dispute as to the relative effect of ray direction and intensity difference by studying the reactions of Volvox in an aquarium in which the light became more intense gradually from one end to the other. Such a distribution of light was pro- duced by placing a hollow prism filled with a mixture of India ink and glycerine-gelatine between the source of light and the aquarium. The India-ink mixture of course absorbed only a little light at the thin end of the prism, but gradually more toward the thicker end. Under these con- ditions the Volvox colonies collected in light of a given intensity. Oltmanns says (1892, p. 195) that when the prism was put between the source of light and a vessel containing colonies which had a given direction of motion, they changed their direction of motion almost instantly and moved toward the region of optimum intensity. Olt- 40 LIGHT AND THE BEHAVIOR OF ORGANISMS manns and others who used this method of producing light of graded intensity assumed that the Hght rays in the aquarium under such conditions were parallel with each other and perpendicular to the side through which they entered, and that the change in direction of motion when the prism was put into place was due not to the direction of the rays but to difference in light intensity. Oltmanns does not make it clear in what sense he uses these terms. He does not say whether he means difference of intensity in the field or difference on the surface of the organism, ray direction in the field or ray direction through the organism. No matter, however, in which sense these terms were used, the conclusion was not warranted, for it is clear from a theoretical as well as from a practical standpoint, that the rays of light, in the aquarium were neither parallel with each other nor perpendicular to the side through which they entered. The India-ink mixture contains numerous solid particles of carbon in suspension, which, together with particles in suspension in the water in the aquarium, unquestionably diffuse the light in such a way that the rays in the aquarium coming from the more highly illuminated end are more numerous than those coming from the other end, and so if the direction of the rays were the control- ling factor one might expect the organisms to go toward either end. After reviewing the work of the preceding authors and presenting some original experiments similar in method to those of Strasburger, Davenport (1897) agrees with Loeb in assuming two dissimilar sorts of locomotor responses to light. These he designates phototaxis and photopathy. Phototaxis he defines " as migration in the direction of the light rays, and photopathy as migration toward a region of greater or less intensity of light." He accepts the theory of orientation as outlined by Sachs and formulates another which is in all essentials like that of Loeb. He says (p. 209) : " Let us first think of the way in which light acts on the negatively phototactic (and photopathic?) earthworm. HISTORICAL REVIEW 4 1 Represent the worm by an arrow whose head Indicates the head end [Fig. 3, ^]. Let solar rays 55 fall upon it horizontally and perpendicularly to its axis. Then the S ii Low Light Attunetnent A <- . Low Light Attunem.ent Fig. 3. Diagram representing sunlight (SS) falling upon an elongated, bilateral organism (represented by the arrow) whose head is at ^. After Davenport (1897, p. 209). impinging ray strikes it laterally, or, in other words, it is illuminated on one side and not on the other. Since, now, the protoplasm of both sides is attuned to an equal intensity of light, that which is the less illuminated is nearer Its optimum intensity. Its protoplasm is in a phototonic con- dition. That which is strongly Illuminated has lost its phototonic condition. Only the darkened muscles, then, are capable of normal contraction; the brightly illuminated ones are relaxed. Under these conditions the organism curves towards the darker side; and since its head region is the most sensitive, response begins there. Owing to a continuance of the causes, the organism will continue to turn from the light until both sides are equally illumi- nated ; i.e. until it is in the light ray. Subsequent locomotion will carry the organism in a straight line, since the muscles of the two sides now act similarly. Thus orientation of the organism is effected. The same ex- planation . . . will account, mutatis mutandis, for positive phototaxis." It Is evident that this theory assumes a direct effect of the stimulating agent on the locomotor organs. Daven- port thus claims that orientation may be brought about in two ways: " Light acts directly either through difference in intensity on the two sides of the organism, or by the 42 LIGHT AND THE BEHAVIOR OF ORGANISMS course the rays take through the organism " (p. 210). He assumes that changes of Hght intensity do not result in orientation but that stimulation caused by such changes may determine the position of organisms in the field in some such way as described by Engelmann. He says (p. 211), " Two kinds of effects are produced by light: one by the direction of its ray — phototactic; the other by the difference in illumination of parts of the organism — photopathic." Holt and Lee (1901) studied the behavior of Stentor coeruleus in an aquarium receiving light through a prism similar to the one used by Oltmanns, and found that the animals collected at the darker end. In conclusion they support Verworn's theory; but from the preceding dis- cussion of the effect of the prism on the direction of rays it is evident that the validity of this conclusion is questionable. Radl's work on reactions to light was almost entirely confined to the Crustacea and insects. In 1903 after a rather extensive review and criticism of the results and theories of others, and an exposition of his own work, he arrived at two conclusions which are of interest in this connection. One has reference to the mechanics of orien- tation, the other to the explanation of negative reactions. His theory of orientation is based on the conception that change in the direction of motion is brought about by unequal stimulation of symmetrical points on the surface of the organism, a conception which lies at the foundation of all the theories thus far presented, excepting that of Sachs and the first one of Loeb. While all of these differ in some respects, they are alike in that they assume the external agent to act through the effect of chemical changes in the organism. Radl proposes to explain orientation as the direct effect of light on the organism. He says (1903, p. 151) : " Alle Autoren, welche bisher dieses Thema beriihrt haben, haben an indirekte Wirkungen des Lichtes gedacht, dass namlich durch dasselbe chemische Veranderungen HISTORICAL REVIEW 43 hervorgerufen werden, welche erst die Reaktionen des Organismus direkt beelnflussen. . . . Gegeniiber diesen Anschauungen mochte ich das Problem des Phototropismus als direkte Wirkung des Lichtstrahls auf den Organismus auflfassen. Wenn wir namlich konsequent unsere x\uffas- sung der Orientierungserscheinungen durchfiihren woUen, so miissen wir auch den Phototropismus als Folgeerschein- ung aus dem Spiel zweier Krafte, einer ausseren und einer inneren auffassen — ich bemiihe mich wenigstens umsonst mir vorzustellen, dass die Sache anders sein konnte. Die aussere Kraft ist in diesem Falle der Lichtstrahl; derselbe muss eine Druckkraft auf den Organismus ausiiben, ich glaube eine ahnliche Druckkraft, wie auf uns etwa der Luftstrom driickt. Diese Vorstellung scheint recht phan- tastisch zu sein, ich sehe jedoch keinen anderen Ausweg. Es ist nicht notig, dass dieser Druck gross sei, er kann sehr fein sein, aber ein Druck, welcher eine Richtung hat, muss es sein, wenn iiberhaupt eine Orientierung, eine Drehung entstehen kann." The maximum pressure of direct sunlight having an intensity of 5000 ± candle meters is only 0.4 mg. on one square meter of black surface, and only twice as great on an equal area of reflecting surface. According to this theory then, an organism responding to o.i candle meter would have to be stimulated by light not to exceed 0.000016 mg. In view of this fact it is not likely that this theory will ever be seriously considered. It has been presented here merely as a matter of historical interest. Radl's view as to the difference between positive and negative reactions is equally untenable. He concludes his discussion on this subject with the following paragraph (1903, p. 103): '*Ich glaube nun, dass der Unterschied zwischen positivem und negativem Phototropismus ahnlich wie beim Menschen nicht ein Unterschied in der Orien- tierung, sondern nur in der Lokomotion ist; dass das Tier in beiden Fallen gegen die Lichtquelle gleich orientiert ist, jedoch nicht gleiche Muskeln spannt." It is of course 44 LIGHT AND THE BEHAVIOR OF ORGANISMS well known that contrary to Radl's conclusion, most of the organisms which face the source of stimulation when positive, turn and face in the opposite direction when negative. 2. Afore thorough Experimental Analysis Showing the Rela- tive Importance of Internal and External Factors in Behavior None of the investigators thus far mentioned studied the behavior of lower organisms in sufficient detail to be able to tell from direct observation precisely what takes place in the reactions. It was well known from direct observa- tion that many of these organisms form dense aggregations under certain conditions and that they frequently orient when subjected to certain stimuli; but just what takes place during the process of aggregation and orientation was with a few exceptions known only theoretically. Jennings was the first to supply this deficiency in obser- vation. He began his investigations on this subject in 1897 by working out in minutest detail precisely what movements are involved in the formation of the dense aggregations so frequently seen in cultures containing paramecia. His work differs from that of his predecessors in this line largely in that, while they, with the possible exception of Engelmann, studied mass movements and end results, he studied the individuals. He was interested not so much in the aggregations as in the process of their formation. How does each individual get there? and why does it stay there? were prominent questions in his mind. The observations on the formation of aggregations of paramecia were followed by similar observations on the reactions of representative species of the various groups of protozoa and lower metazoa to various sorts of stimuli. All of this work is characterized by unity of purpose, keen- ness of observation and simplicity of method. The results of all of Jennings' work, published in nu- HISTORICAL REVIEW 45 merous papers, were brought together and systematized in the well known book on the " Behavior of Lower Organ- isms " (1906). I shall refer to this book almost exclusively in trying to present his views on the factors involved in the phenomena in which we are especially interested — aggre- gation in regions of given light intensity, orientation and change in sense of reaction. Aggregation in a region having a given light intensity may be formed, according to Jennings, in either of two ways. (i) The organisms get into the region just as they would into any other region, merely by swimming about in an aimless manner, without orientation and without direct movement toward the region. When they get to the limit of the region and are about to pass out into light of a different intensity the sudden change to which they are subjected produces a stimulation which causes a definite reaction. This reaction consists chiefly In a sudden turn toward a given side, frequently after backing some distance, and procedure on a new course. They respond with this reaction every time they come to the edge of the region and therefore remain In this region. Other individuals behave in the same way and this results In an aggregation. " Motor reflex " was the first term applied to this method of reaction with Its various modifications; later it was designated " motor reaction," and finally " avoiding reaction." The essential feature In the avoiding reaction is the fact that the organism always turns toward the same side regardless of the place of application of the stimulus. The side toward which it turns is determined by Internal factors. Thus It is that the direction of turning bears no definite relation to the position of the source of stimulation. The organism may turn directly toward it or away from It or at any angle to It. The method of aggregation thus described by Jennings for Paramecium is in all essentials like that de- scribed by Engelmann in 1882 and 1883 for Paramecium bursaria, Euglena, Bacterium photometrlcum and other organisms. 46 LIGHT AND THE BEHAVIOR OF ORGANISMS (2) In place of getting into regions of a given light in- tensity by mere wandering movements, organisms may orient and move directly toward such regions, and the avoiding reaction may keep them in this region just as described above, or they may remain because it is illumi- nated by light of optimum intensity. If they get into light of lower intensity they become positive and return to the optimum directly after becoming oriented. If they get into light of higher intensity they become negative and orient in the opposite direction, which again causes them to return to the optimum intensity. The organ- ism usually tries numerous positions before it becomes oriented. Many errors are made before the successful posi- tion is attained; many directions of motion are tried; one is selected. Jennings has designated this method of orien- tation as orientation by " trial and error," or more recently merely by ''trial." Some seem to be of the opinion that the trial movements are haphazard movements, that they are not definitely determined. In answer to this Jennings says (1906a, p. 452): ''The behavior may perhaps be most accurately characterized as ' selection from among the conditions produced by varied movements.' In general we find that many organisms are so constituted that internal conditions (permanent or temporary) will produce under stimulation movements that are varied in precisely such a way as to subject the creature to as varied environmental conditions as possible, and thus give it an opportunity to select what is nearest the optimum. Every one of these movements is, of course, as absolutely determined as the most orthodox tropism, only the determining factor is not the localization of the stimulus (or other external factor) alone. " Certain recent writers have seemed to imply that there is a contrast between the 'trial and error' method, and behavior that is definitely determined by structural and other internal conditions. It needs to be emphasized, perhaps, that the behavior which I and others have char- HISTORICAL REVIEW 47 acterized by this phrase is very precisely determined by structural and other internal conditions; indeed, its dis- tinguishing feature is the fact that it is thus determined by such conditions, rather than exclusively by the external conditions." Jennings places particular emphasis on the idea that " activity does not require present external stimulation." This is an idea of which Darwin made much in his work on movement in plants. To explain orientation, Darwin said, we do not need to account for movement; it is only neces- sary to account for change in the direction of movement. Jennings applies this idea to the orientation of animals. The animals are in motion; the question is, how is the direction of motion regulated so as to result in orientation? He says that in many of the infusoria it is regulated by means of the avoiding reaction. "This reaction" (1906, p. 79) "consists in successively 'trying' not only different directions of locomotion, but also different positions of the body axis. As soon therefore as a position is reached in which the disturbance causing the reaction no longer exists, the reaction of course stops; the animal therefore retains this axial position." It will thus be seen that orientation in these forms is, according to Jennings, not brought about by a direct turn- ing of the anterior end of the body toward or away from the source of stimulation. It is not due to unequal stimu- lation of points symmetrically situated on the body; the external agent does not act constantly as a directive stimu- lus. " The position of orientation is not one in which a median plane of symmetry takes up a definite position with reference to the external agent." Not all reactions resulting in orientation are however of this sort. Many organisms have the power of turning directly toward or aw.ay from the side stimulated; in these orientation may take place directly, as Jennings clearly states in the follow- ing words (1906, p. 271), " In the symmetrical Metazoa we of course find many cases in which the animal turns 48 LIGHT AND TH'E BEHAVIOR OF ORGANISMS directly toward or away from the source of stimulation, without anything in the nature of preliminary trial move- ments." Reactions which show a definite relation to the localization of the stimulus '' include perhaps the greater number of the directed movements of the organisms." It is evident, judging from these quotations, that Jen- nings does not hold that all organisms orient by means of avoiding reactions. He does not oppose the idea of direct orientation by means of differential response to localized stimulation. He opposes the view that this is the only method of orientation and the view that orientation is caused by the direct effect of the external agent on the locomotor organs. He holds that the power of differential response to localized stimulation is derived from other methods of reaction, as described in the following quota- tions and abstracts (1906, pp. 306-308): "First we have the simple phenomenon that when a portion of an organism is stimulated this portion may respond by contraction, extension, or other change of movement." Such local reponses to local stimulation we find in Amoeba, Hydra, Sagartia, flatworms and many other soft-bodied animals, and even in man when the electrode of a batter}^ is applied directly over a muscle. " In many cases we find that the relation of the movement to the source of stimulation is brought about indirectly through selection from among varied movements. The organism tries moving in many directions, till it finds one in which there is no stimulus to further change. ... In still other cases the reaction shows a definite relation to the localization of the stimulus, yet it is not due to local reaction of the part stimulated, nor is it brought about by trial. If an infusorian is stimu- lated at the anterior end it swims backward; stimulated at the posterior end it swims forward. Both these move- ments are reactions of the entire organisms, all the motor organs of the body concurring to produce them; they are not produced by local reactions of the organs at one end or the other. . . . Such behavior apparently represents HISTORICAL REVIEW 49 not a primitive condition, but a product of development." " To a change leading away from the optimum (in either plus or minus direction)" the organism responds in such a way as to tend to return to the optimum. " Thus are pro- duced the so-called positive and negative reactions." The essential characteristics in behavior, as analyzed by Jennings, are clearly set forth in the following quotations (1906, pp. 283-292). Internal factors: ** Activity does not require present external stimulation. . . . Activity may change without external cause. . . . Changes in activity depend on changes in physiological states. . . . Reactions to external agents depend on physiological states. . . . The physiological state may be changed by progressive internal processes, particularly those of metabo- lism. . . . The physiological state may be changed by the action of external agents. . . . The physiological state may be changed by the activity of the organism. . . . External agents cause reaction by changing the physio- logical state of the organism. . . . The behavior of the organism at any moment depends upon its physiological state at that moment. . . . Physiological states change in accordance with certain laws. . . . The resolution of one physiological state into another becomes easier and more rapid after it has taken place a number of times." Different factors on which behavior depends : "We have seen that the behavior of the organism at a given moment depends on its physiological state, and that it therefore secondarily depends upon all the factors upon which the physiological state depends. Hence we cannot expect the behavior to be determined alone by the present external stimulus, as is sometimes maintained, for this is only one factor in determining the physiological state. The be- havior at a given moment may depend on the following factors, since these all affect the physiological state of the organism : " I. The present external stimulus. ''2. Former stimuli. 50 LIGHT AND THE BEHAVIOR OF ORGANISMS 3. Former reactions of the organism. 4. Progressive internal changes (due to metabolic pro- cesses, etc.). ''5. The laws of the resolution of physiological states one into another. " All these factors have been strictly demonstrated by observation and experiment, even in unicellular organisms. Any one of these alone, or any combination of these, may determine the activity at a given moment." External factors (p. 299): "We may sum up the external factors that produce or determine reactions as follows: (i) The organism may react to a change, even though neither beneficial nor injurious. (2) Anything that tends to inter- fere with the normal current of life activities produces reactions of a certain sort ('negative'). (3) Any change that tends to restore or favor the normal life processes may produce reactions of a different sort ('positive'). (4) Changes that in themselves neither interfere with nor assist the normal stream of life processes may produce negative or positive reactions, according as they are usually followed by changes that are injurious or beneficial. (5) Whether a given change shall produce reaction or not, often depends on the completeness or incompleteness of the performance of the metabolic processes of the organism under the exist- ing conditions. This makes the behavior fundamentally regulatory." Reactions and change in the sense of reactions are, therefore, according to Jennings, adaptive ; and if this be true, an explanation of them must be looked for along the same lines as an explanation of any other adaptive charac- teristic in organisms, functional as well as structural. Finally we may refer to the "selection of random move- ments" as a factor in orientation, as put forward by Holmes (1905). He studied the reactions to light of earth- worms and blow-fly larvae and found that when these animals are stimulated they turn in many directions, apparently feeling about until they become directed away HISTORICAL REVIEW 51 from the source of stimulation. From these observations he concluded that "orientation is produced indirectly by following up these chance movements which bring respite from the stimulation." This conclusion is in perfect harmony with that of Jen- nings regarding the orientation of protozoa. The only difference between the orienting reactions in the two classes of animals mentioned is that the unicellular forms studied by Jennings turn in different directions by means of the avoiding reaction, i.e., they always turn toward a struc- turally defined side, while the metazoa investigated by Holmes are not thus limited in their direction of turning. Not all protozoa however are limited in the direction of turning. Lacrymaria olar, for example, swings its long anterior proboscis-like appendage about in all directions and there appears to be no limitation set to the direction in which it may turn. Holmes contrasts the random movements w^ith forced reflexes, and characterizes the former as " elements of spontaneous, undirected activity." This statement natu- rally leads to the conclusion that the direction of motion in random movements is not definitely determined. It is however hardly probable that Holmes intends to convey such an idea, for it is undoubtedly true that the direction in random movements is as definitely and absolutely deter- mined as it is in the avoiding reaction or in forced reflexes. The difference is merely that the factors involved are different in the different methods of reaction. 3. Summary of Historical Review (i) During the early periods of civilized man all living things were held to be endowed with a soul which was responsible for all activity. (2) Mechanical explanations of activity received but little attention until early in the seventeenth century, the period of Harvey, Descartes and Borelli. 52 LIGHT AND THE BEHAVIOR OF ORGANISMS (3) This period resulted in the origin of the iatromechani- cal and iatrochemical schools. The object of these schools was to explain all vital phenomena on purely physical and chemical principles. (4) The failure to accomplish this purpose led to the origin of the doctrine of vital force, during the first years of the eighteenth century. This resulted in a period of stagnation in research in this line which continued until the appearance of Johannes Miiller, De CandoUe and many others, early in the nineteenth century. (5) The establishment of the doctrine of evolution by Darwin and the consequent interest in the origin of mental phenomena in man led to special activity in the study of behavior of animals from the psychological point of view, and numerous anthropomorphic explanations of their activity. (6) In plants activity was studied from the physico- chemical point of view during this period. This study resulted in the development of the idea that the actions are definitely controlled by external agents, e.g., the direc- tion of growth in roots and stems by gravity, moisture, light, etc. The reactions thus definitely controlled were called tropisms. At first the term tropism was used merely to indicate the relation between the direction of bending and the position of the source of stimulation (De Candolle, 1832). Tropisms were however in general regarded as reactions unequivocally controlled by external agents. (7) The study of animal behavior from the physico- chemical point of view was first taken up by Verworn and Loeb in 1886 and 1887. The activity of the different organs in animals had been studied from this point of view for nearly three centuries, but not the reactions of the animal as a whole. Loeb attempted to show that the behavior In plants and animals is essentially the same, and concluded that the behavior of animals is very largely un- equivocally controlled by external agents. He and his followers therefore described reactions in animals in terms ^ HISTORICAL REVIEW 53 of tropisms in opposition to the anthropomorphic descrip- tions current at that time. Animals go toward a source of light neither because it is useful for them to do so nor because they enjoy light or can see, but because they are positively heliotropic. But what is the underlying cause of tropisms? What are the mechanics involved in the processes described by this term? Loeb applied the theo- ries developed by botanists to answer these questions and developed others (see p. 25). Verworn and other in- vestigators added new ones or suggested modifications. Thus it came about that the term tropism came to have a multiplicity of meanings. (8) Some of the explanations of behavior offered under the name tropism were founded on the idea that the external agent acts directly or through a direct reflex mechanism on the locomotor organs. This idea together with others assuming unequivocal control of behavior by external factors, Jennings and his followers found to be untenable in their studies on the behavior of the lower organisms. The new features introduced by this school have been clearly set forth above ; it will therefore not be necessary to emphasize them here. 4. Various Definitions of Tropisms The term tropism was first used by De Candolle in 1832. He called the bending of plants toward the light helio- tropism, indicating merely the relation between the direc- tion of bending and the source of stimulation. Later the term tropism came to signify not only the bending or orient- ing but also the explanation of the process. Thus for every new explanation the term received a new signification, and this has naturally led to much confusion. Let us point out some of the different meanings which have been applied to the term heliotropism. (i) Sachs in 1876 concluded, as stated above, that orientation of plants is due not to difference in light inten- 54 LIGHT AND THE BEHAVIOR OF ORGANISMS sity on the surface as De Candolle held, but to the direction in which the rays pass through the tissue. HeHotropism, to some of those who agreed with Sachs, meant orientation due to direction of rays through the tissue, to others merely orientation due to ray direction in general. (2) Darwin in 1880 said orientation in plants is due to modification of circumnutation. It is regulated by differ- ence of intensity on opposite surfaces, probably changes of intensity, and he used the term heliotropism to indicate this. (3) In 1888 Loeb maintained that orientation in animals is controlled by the direction in which the rays pass through the tissue, that is, in the same way in which Sachs had said it was controlled in plants. In 1889 he still held that light reactions in plants and animals are governed by the same lawa. But now he says symmetrically located points on the photosensitive surface must be struck by light at the same angle. " Light automatically puts the plant or the animal into such a position that the axis of symmetry of the body, or organ, falls into the direction of the rays of light." Heliotropism is however used not only to express this explanation of orientation, which differs materially from that of Sachs, but also to indicate movement toward or from the source of light. In his later work, he abandons the idea of the importance of the angle between the sen- sitive surface and the light rays and substitutes the idea that it is relative intensity on opposite sides which governs orientation. Thus heliotropism received a new significa- tion. His most recent views are expressed in the following quotations (1906, pp. 135, 138): " Heliotropism covers only those cases where the turning to light is compulsory and irresistible, and is brought about automatically or mechani- cally by the light itself. ... If the current curves of radiating energy, e.g., light rays, strike an animal on one side only, or on one side more strongly than on the sym- metrical side, the velocity or the kind of chemical reactions in the symmetrical photosensitive points of both sides of HISTORICAL REVIEW 55 the body will be different. The consequence will be in a positively heliotropic animal a stronger tension or tendency to contract in the muscles connected with the photosensitive points of the one side of the body than in those connected with the opposite side." This view is affirmed in a recent address (1909). (4) It is ordinarily assumed that Verworn"- considers orientation in the lower forms to be due to the direct effect ' of the external agent on the locomotor appendages. If, e.g.t one side is more highly illuminated than the other the cilia beat more or less effectively on that side and thus produce orientation. This process is termed heliotropism or phototaxis. (5) " Two kinds of effects are produced by light " accord- ing to Davenport (1907, pp. 210, 211), " one by the direc- tion of the rays . . . either through difference of intensity on the two sides of the organism, or by the course the rays take through the organism — phototactic; the other by the difference in illumination of parts of the organism — photopathic." (6) Yerkes says (1903, p. 361), "All those reactions in which the direction of movement is determined by an orientation of the organism which is brought about by the light are phototactic; and all those reactions in which the movement, although due to the stimulation of light, is not definitely directed through the orientation of the organism are photopathic." (7) To Radl (1903) heliotropism means orientation due to difference in light pressure on unequally illuminated symipetrically located surfaces. (8) Holmes (1905) calls orientation by selection of ran- dom movements phototaxis (heliotropism). (9) Barrows (1907, p. 530) and Walter (1907, p. 149) suggest "asymmetrical response to asymmetrical stimula- tion" as a criterion of tropisms; and because the organisms worked on respond thus they conclude that their reactions are tropic. According to this criterion it is of course evident 56 LIGHT AND THE BEHAVIOR OF ORGANISMS that every diflferentlal response to a localized stimulation even in a human being may be a tropic response. (10) To Bohn forced orientation constitutes a tropism; (1908, p. 78), " L' orientation est directe; I'animal est attire sans qu'il pitisse resister: il y a la un ' tropisme' au sens de Loeb "; (p. 80), " On n'a pas besoin de nier la 'volonte' de Tanimal; on peut dire que ces impulsions sont plus fortes qu'elle. On ne peut nier les tropismes." (11) Parker apparently considers any reaction which carries an animal toward or away from the source of stimu- lation as tropic; he says (1908, p. 426), " Since amphioxus swims away from a source of light, it is negatively photo- tropic." Minkiewicz (1907, p. 47), uses the term tropism in much the same sense, as does also Hadley, who defines it and photopathy as follows (1908, p. 201) : " A phototactic reaction [is] one in w^hich the organism tends to place the longitudinal axis of the body parallel to the direction of the rays and to approach or recede from the source of those rays. ... A photopathic reaction is one in w^hich an or- ganism, without previous assumption of a body-orientation, ^selects' regions of optimal light-intensity." (12) Washburn (1908, p. 57) refers to tropisms as "the direct motor response of an animal to an external stimulus," and Torrey defines the term similarly but somewhat more definitely. He says (1907, p. 319): *' In heliotropism as well as in galvanotropism, the oriented organism is in a condition of physiological stimulation, and . . . the re- sponse to stimulation is local." This definition is in all essentials like those of Verworn and Loeb. (13) Driesch (1908, p. 11) says, "A tropism ... is a directed movement of a growing part of a plant or hydroid determined by the direction of a directed agent." (14) Wheeler (1910, p. 515) considers reactions which *' involve an adaptive orientation" as tropic. (15) Jennings (1909, p. i) suggests the following defini- tion: " The tropism includes those reactions in which the organism takes and maintains a definite orientation — places HISTORICAL REVIEW 57 the axis of its body in a definite position — with relation to some external source of stimulation." It is evident from these statements that nearly every reaction in living organisms comes under one or another of the various definitions given to the term tropism. To say that an organism is tropic or not tropic means but little until the sense in which this term is used is defined. Failure to do this has led to serious misunderstanding. I have no objection whatever to the term tropism if used in its original sense, or in any other definite sense. At present, however, it conveys so many different meanings that it inevitably leads to confusion. I shall therefore avoid using it in the following analysis of reactions to light. 5. Statement of Important Problems in the Study of Reactions to Light In this analysis we shall ai n to keep in mind the various factors suggested as important in the different tropism theories and other explanations of behavior. We shall ask ourselves: is orientation direct, does the organism turn directly toward or away from the source of stimulation, or does it become oriented after a series of preliminary move- ments? How is the stimulus causing orientation pro- duced: by direction of rays through the organism in accord with the theory of Sachs; by absolute difference of intensity on symmetrically located points on the sensitive surface in accord with the theories of Loeb and Verworn; or by changes of intensity on the surface in accord with the ideas of Engel- mann, Darwin, and Jennings? Does light act constantly as a directive stimulation similar to the action of a constant current of electricity in accord with Loeb's theory of trop- ism, or does it act only when the organism turns out of its course so as to produce changes of intensity, as suggested by Jennings? Is orientation due to the direct effect of light on the locomotor appendages in accord with the theory of Verworn and the analysis of Torrey, to the indi- 58 LIGHT AND THE BEHAVIOR OF ORGANISMS rect effect through a direct reflex arc as suggested by Loeb, or is the whole organism more or less involved in the re- action in accord with the ideas of Jennings and Holmes? If orientation is direct, precisely what movements are involved in the process? Are the avoiding reactions due to differential response to localized stimulation, as held by some, or is the direction of turning in such reactions abso- lutely determined by the structure and physiological state of the organism? Are the reactions to light in general adaptive and modifiable in accord with Jennings' analysis, or are they fixed and forced and unequivocally controlled by the external agent in accord with Loeb's ideas? Are the more refrangible rays most active in stimulating all organisms as claimed by Loeb and Davenport, or are some organisms stimulated more by waves of a certain length, and others by waves of a different length as claimed by Verworn and Nagel? These questions and others we shall attempt to answer in the following pages. PART II EXPERIMENTAL OBSERVATIONS AND DISCUS- SIONS BEARING ON THE QUESTION AS TO HOW ORGANISMS (ESPECIALLY THOSE WITHOUT EYES) BEND OR TURN AND MOVE TOWARD OR FROM A SOURCE OF STIMULATION CHAPTER IV PROCESSES INVOLVED IN THE BENDING OF DIFFERENT PARTS OF HIGHER PLANTS TOWARD THE SOURCE OF LIGHT I . Observations on Plumules of Indian Corn {Zea mays) and Leaves of Nasturtium ( Tropaeolum) a. Introduction. — It is well known that many plant structures have a sensitive zone which may be separated by some distance from the motory zone and that impulses are transmitted from the one to the other. Darwin (1880), Pfeffer (1894), Czapek (1900), Pollock (1900), Haberlandt (1904) and others demonstrated this for leaves and plumules stimulated by light and for radicles stimulated by gravita- tion and injury (cauterization). Newcombe (1902, p. 346) also proved that impulses due to stimulation by water currents are transmitted in radicles. In radicles the dis- tance of transmission of impulses is frequently over 10 mm., while in leaves it is often several centimeters. Just how the external agent produces the stimulus is not known, although it is generally supposed that it is by caus- ing chemical changes. With regard to light it has been a question as to whether the orienting stimulation is depend- 59 6o LIGHT AND THE BEHAVIOR OF ORGANISMS ent upon the direction in which the rays pass through the tissue or upon difference of intensity on opposite sides of the reacting organ. Sachs (see p. 13) originated the former view and Miiller and others supported it, while Darwin, Wiesner and Oltmanns were prominent champions of the latter. Darwin also emphasized in particular the impor- tance of change in intensity. Pfeffer (1906, p. 228) says that the experimental results and the arguments offered in support of either view are not conclusive. Darwin exposed monocot plumules (stems of young seedlings) with one side covered with India ink in front of a window and found that they did not bend straight toward the window, but deflected toward the uncovered side. This result seems to indicate that the curvature is due to differ- ence in light intensity on the surfaces. Pfeffer (1906, pp. 3, 229), however, considers it inconclusive, largely on account of the possible effect of the India ink on transpiration (evaporation). Oltmanns studied the curvature of plants grown behind a hollow prism containing India ink and glycerine gelatine so arranged that the light intensity decreased from right to left, and found that they deflected toward the brighter end of the field. He therefore con- cluded in favor of difference of intensity as the controlling factor in orientation. His results, however, are not con- clusive, owing to the diffusion of light by the particles of India ink in suspension (see p. 40). b. Apparatus. — In the following work the objections to the experiments of Darwin and Oltmanns were elimi- nated by the use of an apparatus known as the light grader modified to suit the conditions of the experiments. The important features in the construction of this apparatus will be understood readily by referring to Fig. 4. The walls of the apparatus are all light-proof and dead black inside, so as to prevent reflection. The outline of a cross section at any point is square. The upper portion of the front wall of the vertical part of the apparatus is hung on hinges forming a door. From the bottom of this door is BENDING OF HIGHER PLANTS TOWARD THE LIGHT 6l- Fig. 4. I. A vertical section of the light grader. The lens (a), which is a seg- ment of a cylinder, has its longitudinal axis lying in the plane of the section; b, stage; c, Nernst glower; d, non-reflecting background; e, mirror;/, light rays; g, opaque screens. Distance from glower of lamp to stage, one meter. II. Stereographic view of light, lens, and image; a, lens; b, field of light pro- duced by the image of the glower (c); d, opaque screen, which lies flat on lens and contains a triangular opening which causes a gradation in the light intensity of the field (b). hung a loose vertical curtain, which can be so opened that observations can be made without admitting Hght. The source of hght Is a Nernst glower, which Is parallel with the minor axis of the lens. It is mounted In front of a small opening In a light-proof box painted dead black Inside, which thus forms a non-reflecting background. The glower 62 LIGHT AND THE BEHAVIOR OF ORGANISMS and stage are at the conjugate focal points of the lens, and therefore at equal distances (50 cm.) from it. The plano- convex cylindrical lens used is 25 cm. long, 10 cm. wide and has a radius of curvature of 12.5 cm. A cylindrical lens will not form a single definite image of an object, but rather a series of images, since by means of it light is focused only in reference to one plane. If, then, the object, e.g., a Nernst glower, is placed at one of the conjugate focal points so that the distance from the lens to the glower is equal to that from the lens to the image, and the glower is so arranged that it is perpendicular to the axis of the lens, the image will not consist of a narrow band of light as large as a glower, which would be true if the segment of a sphere were used as the lens, but it will consist of a comparatively large field of light, the length of which is proportional to the functional length of the lens, while the width is equal to the length of the glower, regard- less of the functional width of the lens (see Fig. 4). But since the amount of light which passes through the lens is directly proportional to the functional width of the lens and the width of the field is constant, it is clear that the intensity of light in the field, if we disregard the amount of light absorbed by the lens, must also be theoretically pro- portional to its functional width. Direct measurements of the light intensity with different functional widths of the lens proved this to be true within the limits of error. If, then, the lens be covered with an opaque screen containing a triangular opening, the base of which is parallel with the minor axis of the lens as represented in Fig. 4, there will result a rectangular field of light in which the intensity gradually diminishes from the end produced by light which passes through the base of the triangular opening to the opposite end, where theoretically it fades into darkness. Practically, however, it was found to be impossible to cut the apex of the triangular opening so as to prevent an apparent line at the end of least intensity. Since the light intensity of the field is proportional to the functional width BENDING OF HIGHER PLANTS TOWARD THE LIGHT 63 of the lens, it Is evident that the rate of diminution in intensity depends upon the ratio of the altitude of the triangular opening to the length of its base; i.e., decreasing the altitude or increasing the base causes an increase in the rate of diminution, and vice versa. Yerkes (1903) was the first to make use of a cylindrical lens in studying re- actions to light. c. Experiments. — In these experiments the light grader was placed in a horizontal position in such a way that the glower was vertical. The lens was covered with an opaque screen containing two triangular openings with the apexes facing each other and only a millimeter apart. In this way two parallel horizontal beams of light were produced, the intensity of which gradually diminished from side to side (see Fig. 5). The object of having two beams was to neutralize any possible effect from diffusion of light by the lens. A single plumule at a time was exposed In one of these beams of light. In some cases it was allowed to grow up into it from a small pot of sphagnum in which It was ger- minated; In others the seedlings were transferred to the light grader after the plumules were about one centimeter long. In former experiments with this apparatus aquatic or- ganisms were used; it was therefore necessary to expose them in an aquarium containing water. Under such con- ditions it is Impossible to eliminate light reflected from the glass walls of the aquarium and from particles in suspension in the water. With the plumule growing in air, however, and with only one exposed in the beam of light at a time, it is evident that all such reflections are done away with. Thus the objections to Oltmanns' experiments with the hollow prism have been obviated, and likewise those brought forward against Darwin's work. All the following experiments were performed in a large dark room. During the first part of the work the apparatus was situated several meters from a dead black wall upon 64 LIGHT AND THE BEHAVIOR OF ORGANISMS which the beams of light fell and were absorbed. The altitude of the triangular openings in the screen over the lens was 7 mm. and the base 50 mm. The beams of light thus produced were 14 mm. wide and 20 mm. high at the focal point in the light grader, the place where the plumules were exposed. At this point the light intensity in each beam decreased from side to side at the rate of 2 ca. m.^ per mm., it being 100 ca. m. at one side and zero at the other. From these data the intensity at any part could readily be calculated. In order to ascertain the intensity to which the plumules w^ere exposed it was therefore neces- sary only to learn their position in the field ; and to calculate the difference of intensity on opposite sides it was sufficient to know their diameter, the difference in all parts of the field being 2 ca. m. per mm. width. During the first part of the work the movements of each plumule were recorded by tracing its shadow cast upon a sheet of paper held in a vertical position a few centimeters back of it. The shadow was thus traced at the beginning of the experiment and again at definite intervals. At first only a few tracings were made in twenty-four hours. It was however soon found that owing to marked circum- nutating movements and to surprisingly indefinite lateral deflections it was necessary to locate the position of the plumules at 30 to 60 minute intervals (see Fig. 5). By this method only the lateral and vertical movements of the plumule were recorded. There was no record of the movement toward the source of light; in some of the later experiments however this movement also w^as recorded. A fine pointer was fastened so that the sharp end was 10 cm. above the tip of the plumule. A glass plate was then fastened in a horizontal position one meter above the pointer. By proper illumination the sharp end of the pointer and the tip of the radicle could clearly be seen throu^^h the glass plate, and it was not difficult to fix a ^ The a bbreviation ca. m. will be used for the term candle meters through- out this vc>lume. \ BENDING OF HIGHER PLANTS TOWARD THE LIGHT 65 dot of ink in line with these on the plate by sighting through a small circular hole in a piece of opaque paper. The horizontal movements of the tip of the radicle could thus be quite accurately recorded by making dots on the plate in line with the pointer and the tip of the radicle, at any -> n -> ■> 13 12 Fig. 5. Tracings of shadow of a plumule of corn showing its reaction in light of graded intensity, three-fourths natural size. I. Cross section of two beams of light as used in the experiment; intensity at a and a', zero; at b and b', 100 ca. m.; I, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7, successive positions of plumule at intervals of 60 minutes, right side more highly illuminated than left; 8, 9, 10, 11, 12, 13, same with left side more highly illuminated than right. It will be seen that the plumules deflect slightly toward the more highly illuminated side under both conditions. II. Side view of plumule showing amount of curvature toward source of light at close of experiment, n, direction of light. desired intervals, and connecting them with a line. The records thus made represent the movement of the radicle magnified ten times. The direction of the rays was recorded by tracing the edge of a ruler placed on the glass plate in such a position that the edge was in line with the shadow of the plumule cast on a white surface temporarily arranged for the purpose (Fig. 6). The intensity of light to which the plumules were ex- posed varied from about 2 to 14 ca. m. In most of the experiments they were exposed to the lowest intensity, the edge of the plumule at the beginning of the experiment jDeing in close contact with that side of the beam of light which had the lowest intensity (see Fig. 5). 66 LIGHT AND THE BEHAVIOR OF ORGANISMS B <^o4^- '"^-^ 9:40 A.M. 11:00 .11:30 12:30 P.M. 1:00 1:55 2:30 d 9:15 A.M:. 10:00 Fig. 6. A-E. Courses taken by tips of plumules in bending toward the glower in a graded beam of light; magnified five times. The dots represent the position at time indicated. The large arrows indicate direction of rays; the small ones the direction of movement of plumules; d, side of the beam having the lowest light intensity; /, side having highest intensity. In E the beam was reversed between i.ooand 2.00 p.m. It will be seen that in every case except A the plumules de- flected slightly toward the more highly illuminated side. See text. d. Results. — Under these conditions the reactions of 36 plumules, 14 of wheat (Triticum vulgare) and 22 of corn (Zea mays), were studied and recorded with the following results: of the 14 wheat plumules studied 6 deflected toward the more highly illuminated side, 3 toward the less highly illuminated side, and 5 did not appreciably deflect in either direction. Of the 22 corn plumules 13 deflected toward th^ more highly illuminated side, 2 toward the less highly BENDING OF HIGHER PLANTS TOWARD THE LIGHT 67 illuminated side, and 7 did not definitely deflect toward either side. These results seem to indicate that it is difi"erence in light intensity on the organism which regulates the direction of movement. The lateral deflections are, however, as indi- cated in Fig. 6, relatively small. The maximum is scarcely more than 2 mm. in a movement of 10 mm. tow^ard the source of light. Considering the conditions of the experi- ments superficially one would expect a much greater deflec- tion if the direction is regulated by the relation in light intensity on different parts of the surface. A corn plumule frequently has a diameter of over one millimeter at a point not more than one millimeter from the tip, well within the sensitive zone. In such a plumule placed in contact with the edge of the beam of light having the lowest intensity, the difference of intensity between the surface facing the glower and that facing in the opposite direction is appar- ently not as great as the difference of intensity between the two sides. Consequently one might conclude that if the movement is regulated by difference of intensity, the plumule should bend at least as far toward the highly illuminated edge of the beam as toward the glower. There are however serious objections to such a conclusion. In the first place it is not known whether or not the sensitive tissue extends to the surface. It may be that it is restricted to the central portion of the plumule and that it is very narrow, so that the intensity difference on opposite sides of this tissue is relatively slight under the conditions of the experiment. In the second place It is evident that light can affect the tissue only by penetrating it, and since the rays strike the surface facing the glower nearly at right angles, and the more highly Illuminated side at a very small angle, much more light will penetrate the former than the latter. And in the third place, under the conditions of the experiment, the Illumination of the two sides will be equalized by the movement of the plumules much sooner than will that of the two surfaces. 6S> LIGHT AND THE BEHAVIOR OF ORGANISMS However this may be, It must be conceded that while the results of these experiments indicate that orientation is due to diversity of light intensity on the reacting organ, they do not definitely settle the question. Much more convincing results were obtained toward the close of the work when it occurred to me that it would be possible to prevent the bending toward the glower entirely, without vitiating the results, by reflecting the beam of light and illuminating the surface directed away from the glower as well as that facing it. A small mirror of finest quality 5 mm. X 2 cm. was therefore supported in the beam of light in a vertical position 3 cm. from the plumule. By careful manipulation and frequent adjustment it was pos- sible to keep the intensity on the surface directed toward the glower and the one opposite nearly the same, while the difference of Intensity on the right and left sides was nearly twice as great as it was when the beam was not reflected. The reactions of 4 plumules of Zea mays were studied under these conditions. All deflected definitely toward the more highly Illuminated side, as represented In Fig. 7. These results seem to prove conclusively that orientation In plu- mules of the gramineae (grasses) is in some way regulated by difference in light Intensity on opposite sides, and that the direction in which the rays enter the tissue Influences the direction of motion only in so far as this may produce unequal illumination of different parts of the sensitive tissue. A number of experiments were made with young nas- turtium (Tropaeolum) leaves In graded light. Different parts of the leaf blades were thus subjected to different intensities. In some experiments one-half of the blade was entirely in the shadow. I was unable to detect any influ- ence of the unequal illumination of the blade on orientation. The leaves turned toward the source of light just as they did when the blades were entirely Illuminated by light of equal Intensity throughout. The circumnutatlon move- ments in these leaves were so great, however, that it would a M ,, + -I y -' 10:io A.M 11;30 5:10 m d 8:05 A.M. %-j l:i5 P.M. 11:25 P.M. He 7.40 5:00 4:30 3:30 a t t " d 12:30 P.M. 1:00' m 1:45 5:15 :05 9-00-11:80 A.M m n o n '1 ;.Vi E F Fig. 7. ^-Z). Courses taken by tips of plumules of Indian corn (Zea mays) as viewed from above in a beam of graded light (a) which was reflected from the mirror m so as to illuminate the two surfaces equally. Magnified five times. The arrows (a) indicate the direction of the rays from the glower, and the other arrows the direction of movement of plumules; d, side of beam of light having lowest intensity; /, side having highest intensity (see F below). Movement in the direc- tion of the rays of light was caused by imperfect adjustment of mirror producing unequal illumination from the glower and mirror. E, Cross section of beams of Hght. i, outline of shadow of plumule at the be- ginning of Course C above, i.io p.m.; 2, shadow at 2.10 p.m.; 3, shadow at 11.25 P-M. F, I, shadow of plumule at beginning of Course D above, 9.00 a.m.; 2, same at 5.15 P.M. The light intensity at 0 was zero; at n, about 200 ca. m. The increase of intensity in the field from side to side was about 14 ca. m. per mm. It will be seen that the plumules deflected strongly toward the side most highly illuminated. 69 70 LIGHT AND THE BEHAVIOR OF ORGANISMS have been impossible to detect anything but rather de- cided effects. It is hoped that these experiments may be extended. e. Discussion. — The conclusion arrived at above that orientation is regulated by the difference in light intensity on opposite sides of the plumules is in direct opposition to Sachs' theory (see p. 13) of orientation. It opposes that of Loeb in so far as he attaches importance to the idea that symmetrically situated points on the surface must be struck by light at the same angle when the organism is oriented (see p. 28). It neither confirms nor contradicts Loeb's and Verworn's idea (see pp. 29, 38) as to the direct effect of the external agent on the motory tissue. Nor does it bear on the question proposed by Darwin (p. 18) that orientation is due exclusively to modification of circum- nutations. It is entirely possible that the lateral illumi- nation causes an increase as well as a change in the direction of the movement. Superficially the evidence seems to indicate clearly that orientation is direct, that there is nothing corresponding to selection of random movements (see p. 50). However, it is impossible to say in how far even very slight circum- nutating changes in position may affect diversity of light intensity within the individual cells in the sensitive zone, and in how far such changes in position may be interpreted as trial movements. Owing to the possibility of such variations in illumination within the cells, due to very slight changes in the position of the plumule, it is also impossible to decide whether the stimuli which cause orientation are due to constant intensity or to change of Intensity. These experiments have no bearing on the question as to how curvature resulting in orientation in the plumules is produced. The experiments of Darwin and others, how- ever, showing that there Is a distinct sensory and motory zone in these structures, demonstrate clearly that it is not due to the direct effect of the illumination on the tissues BENDING OF HIGHER PLANTS TOWARD THE LIGHT 7 1 which produces the curvature, as Loeb's theory quo ted above demands. The mechanism involved is undoubtedly far more complex than this theory indicates. It may be similar to that offered by Pollock to explain the curvatures in roots. He says (1900, p. 59): " The stimulus is transmitted from the sensitive root tip to the curving parts, in the cortical parenchyma. The effect of the stimulus is to increase the normal tension between cortical parenchyma and axial cylin- der on the side that becomes convex, and to decrease or re- verse the normal tension between the cortical parenchyma and the axial cylinder on the side that becomes concave. The change in tension also extends to the different layers of the cortical parenchyma on the concave side, the outer layers becoming negative with respect to the inner ones. So much has been demonstrated. The evidence is in favor of the view that the tensions on the concave side are changed by the protoplasm becoming more permeable to water, some of which passes out into intercellular spaces, possibly to be taken up by the convex cells, which later contain more water than the concave cells. The shorten- ing of the concave side may be masked sometimes by a certain amount of growth." This theory does not account for curvature in structures having but a single cell cavity, like the hyphae of molds, rhizoids of liverw^orts, and some algae, all of which are known to respond to light by bending toward or from its source. That these reactions cannot be accounted for on the basis of osmotic changes was pointed out by Hofmeister as early as 1867. Very little is known concerning the fundamental factors involved in orientation in other plant structures than those mentioned, although much work has been done on them, especially on the leaves. Darwin (1881) was the first to attempt to locate the sensitive structure in the leaf. He found that neither quality nor intensity of reaction is affected by shading the blade, and concluded that the petiole perceives the light. V^oechting (1888) came to quite the opposite conclusion in experiments on malva 72 LIGHT AND THE BEHAVIOR OF ORGANISMS and other plants. Krabbe (1889) supported Darwin in his conclusion, as did also Rothert (1894) and Czapek. Haber- landt (1904), on the other hand, maintains not only that the blade is functional in light perception, but also that the curved and thickened outer walls of the epidermal cells act as lenses and focus the light on the protoplasm within, and that orientation is regulated by responses due to the dis- tribution of the intensity of light within the cells of the epidermis. Kneip (1907) covered the upper surface of the blades of Tropaeolum with a thin layer of paraffin oil whose index of refraction is about 0.143 greater than the index of cell sap. The oil consequently inverted the lens effect of the curved walls of the epidermal cells and thus caused a dispersal of the rays within the cell. Kneip found however that the leaves treated thus responded to light much like those not treated, and concluded (p. 136), '' that the lens action is of no importance in the leaves studied." Haber- landt (1909) however does not agree with this conclusion. He claims that the fact that leaves still respond to light after the epidermis is covered in such a way as to neutralize the focusing effect of the curvature of the outer cell walls, merely shows that the effect of these walls can be dispensed with and not that it is useless, and holds that after the lens effect of these walls is neutralized the light intensity is still unequal on the inner surface of the cells, when the light strikes the epidermis obliquely, and that this may cause orienting responses, but that the focusing effect of the curved outer walls of the cells enhances the promptness and precision of the orienting responses. Various other experiments aside from those mentioned above have been carried out, but the results obtained lead to no definite conclusions concerning the function of the lens action of the epidermal cells, nor do they give any clear notion as to the mechanism of orientation in plants. About all that can be said is that leaves generally take a position such as to facilitate photosynthesis, and that the chloroplasts within the cells likewise assume what may be BENDING OF HIGHER PLANTS TOWARD THE LIGHT 73 termed an optimum position. The reactions are adaptive. In some instances if the Hght is too intense the chloroplasts are found along the side walls which are more or less nearly parallel with the incident rays. In others the leaves turn so that the edge of the blade faces the light. In all proba- bility both the petiole and the blade are sensitive to light, at least in some leaves, but the method of regulating the movements is still a mystery. CHAPTER V OBSERVATIONS ON UNICELLULAR FORMS IN THE PROCESS OF ATTAINING AND RETAINING A DEFINITE AXLA.L POSITION WITH REFERENCE TO THE SOURCE OF LIGHT I. Myxomycetes and Rhizopods All the Rhizopods and the plasmodia of Myxomycetes that are known to react to Hght are negative, as was shown by Baranetzsky (1876, pp. 328, 340), Stahl (1884, p. 167), Engelmann (1879, p. 3), Davenport (1897) and others. The contention of Hofmeister (1867, p. 20) that plasmodia are positive in Hght of very low intensity has not been confirmed. Davenport (1897, p. 186) exposed specimens of Amoeba proteus under a compound microscope in a small horizontal beam of direct sunlight with all other light intercepted by means of opaque screens and found that they orient directly. They make no preliminary trial movements in this process. If the direction of the rays is changed they always turn from the source of light at once, never toward it. There is no evidence of selection of random movements in these animals. The same is probably true in case of other Rhizopods and Myxomycetes, although there are no investigations which bear directly on this point. Baranetzsky (1876) found that even a slight increase in illumination causes a distinct retardation in streaming movements of Myxomycetes. Engelmann (1879) observed that light thrown upon a pseudopod of Pelomyxa palustris causes it to be withdrawn suddenly. Harrington and Leaming (1900) found that a sudden increase in light intensity causes a retardation in the movement of Amoeba. Ewart (1903, p. 69) says that protoplasmic streaming in cells in general is retarded by 74 OBSERVATIONS ON UNICELLULAR FORMS 75 increase in light intensity, and Pringsheim (1879, pp. 334, 367) maintains that local retardations in streaming move- ment can be produced by local stimulation. Jennings (1904) has shown the same to be true for Amoeba when stimulated mechanically and chemically. After completing this part of the manuscript I had the opportunity of observing the orienting reactions in Amoeba proteus in detail, and also the effect of different rays on the reactions. I shall insert a description of the former here; the latter will be discussed in Part IV. In studyingorientation numerous specimens were mounted under a large cover glass supported by a ring of vaseline so as to give them ample room for moving about and to pre- vent the solution from drying up. The specimens thus enclosed could be kept in excellent condition for several days. The observations were made under a compound microscope situated in diffuse daylight without any screen around it. Mirrors were so arranged that two horizontal beams of direct sunlight were reflected upon the stage at right angles to each other after passing through 8 cm. of water to eliminate the heat. Specimens exposed in one of these beams without any light from the substage were found to direct their course in a general way from the source of light. In one instance, after a slide had been exposed for fifteen minutes, there were eleven specimens in one field of the low power, all but two of which were moving from the source of light. In another field there w^ere twelve specimens; all but four of these were directed from the source of light. Of these four, two were proceeding at right angles to the rays and two were going toward the light. In still another field containing nine specimens, seven were negatively oriented, one positively and one at right angles to the rays. Orientation, however, was not very precise in any of the specimens. The amoebae usually took a sort of zigzag course. Pseudopods were frequently seen to extend toward one side for some distance, then stop as though they had been checked, after which new ones were 76 LIGHT AND THE BEHAVIOR OF ORGANISMS ordinarily seen to extend on the opposite side for some dis- tance, and stop, etc. The details in the process of orientation were observed as follows: a specimen which had oriented in one beam of light was selected, after which the light in this beam was intercepted and that in the other simultaneously turned on. The reaction of numerous specimens to a change in the direction of the rays was thus observed and the move- ments in several were recorded by means of camera sketches made at short intervals. A typical record is presented in Fig. 8, although a majority of the specimens observed did not orient as precisely and definitely as did the one represented in this record. By referring to Fig. 8 it will be seen that the amoeba under observation gradually turned from the side most highly illuminated, sending out pseudo- pods only on the shaded side. What is the cause of this? If direct sunlight is thrown upon an amoeba which is active in diffuse daylight, all movement stops instantly, but there is ordinarily no immediate contraction of any of the pseudopods. After a few moments of exposure new pseudopods usually appear at the posterior end, and not until these begin to form do the old ones begin to retract. In changing the direction of the rays so that the amoebae become strongly illuminated from the side, as described above, the distribution of the light intensity on the different pseudopods is changed since different surfaces become exposed. Judging from our preceding statement it might be expected that this change of light intensity would inhibit the protoplasmic streaming in the pseudopods on the illuminated side. I could, however, never be quite certain that it did, although it often appeared so. The difficulty in observation here lies in the fact that without any change of illumination the pseudopods form, extend a varying distance, then stop and retract while others form elsewhere. When a pseudopod stops after the direction of the rays is changed it is consequently impossible to be certain that it would not have stopped had the light not OBSERVATIONS ON UNICELLULAR FORMS 77 3:48 P.M. 3:54 8:57.5 0.5 mm: FiG. 8. Camera drawing representing different stages in the process of orien- tation of Amoeba proteus. i, Amoeba oriented in light nn before light //is turned on; 2-9 successive positions at the time indicated on each after light II is turned on. Arrows represent the direction of streaming of protoplasm in pseudopods. In those which do not contain arrows there was no noticeable streaming at the time the sketch was made. // and nn, direction of light. 78 LIGHT AND THE BEHAVIOR OF ORGANISMS been changed. If light acts directly on the protoplasm it might also be expected that in a pseudopod laterally illuminated, the flow on one side would be retarded, thus causing it to curve. But no evidence of this could be seen. How then does orientation take place if the pseudopods which are present continue and do not turn from the source of light? There is but one way that I can see, and that is by the inhibition of the formation of new ones on the more highly illuminated side of the organism. Since we know that an increase of intensity inhibits streaming in the pseudopods of Amoeba it seems strange that no one has thus far been able to see any reaction in an amoeba in passing from a region of one intensity to that of another. Davenport (1897, p. 186) studied their move- ments in a field " separated by a sharp line into a light and dark half," but could detect '* no effect resulting from the change from light to dark or the reverse." I made ob- servations much like those of Davenport, and found that when the amoebae came in contact with the light area they usually stopped and proceeded in a different direction, as represented in Fig. 9. The light area used in these experiments was about 0.5 mm. square and had very definite edges and a high intensity. It was produced by focusing a limited area of a luminous Welsbach mantle on the slide by means of the mirror and an Abbe condenser. These observations were made in a dark room and no light except the small beam from the Welsbach rrantle reached the microscope. By referring to Fig. 9 it will be seen that after one pseudopod came in contact with the illumination and was stopped, the amoeba did not at once proceed in the opposite direction so as to avoid the light, but sent out other pseu- dopods at only a slight angle with the first, apparently trying to get around the obstacle in this way. The char- acter of the response did not change after the first pseudopod came in contact with the light, or after the second and the third came in contact with it. But after the fourth became OBSERVATIONS ON UNICELLULAR FORMS 79 exposed the direction of motion was nearly reversed. This indicates that the reaction was modified, that the response to a given stimulus depends upon the preceding experience. Fig. g. Sketches representing the reactions of an amoeba proceeding toward an intense area of light the rays of which were perpendicular to the slide. L, field of light formed by focusing a section of a Welsbach mantle on the slide, i-io, successive positions of the amoeba a little less than one-half minute apart. Arrows indicate direction of streaming in pseudopods. In view of these facts it is probably true that the orienta- tion of all of the rhizopods in light is due to a local response to a local stimulation, a direct inhibition of the movement of the part most highly illuminated. This would of course result in the prevention of the formation of pseudopods on the more highly illuminated side, and the organism would 8o LIGHT AND THE BEHAVIOR OF ORGANISMS turn until both sides are equally illuminated, and symmet- rically located points on the body equally stimulated. Such a method of orientation is in harmony with much in Verworn's theory and also with the essentials in Loeb's. It does not, however, support the idea connected with these theories, that a constant intensity produces a constant directive stimulation. Jennings (1904) has shown that certain amoebae roll over and over in their movement. The protoplasm on the underside in relatively low light intensity is constantly coming to the surface into a greater intensity, and moreover the beginning of every laterally directed pseudopod in those forms which do not roll necessarily causes a change in the light intensity of the protoplasm in it. Thus it is clear that the protoplasm is being continuously subjected to changes of intensity. And while the rate of movement in the animal as a whole is no doubt influenced by constant light inten- sity, much as it is by temperature, it may be that orienting reactions are responses solely to changes in light intensity, — in negative organisms to a rather sudden increase of intensity. This method of orientation is opposed to the idea of Sachs (see p. 14), that the direction in which the rays penetrate the tissue is of importance in orientation, and also to that of Loeb (see p. 28) with reference to the im- portance of the angle between the rays and the surface. 2. Euglena a. Description. — Euglena is a minute elongated or- ganism. The posterior extremity ends in a spinelike process; the anterior end is rounded off rather bluntly. The different species vary greatly in size; some are not over o.oi mm. long and o.ooi mm. in diameter, while others are nearly fifty times as large. The forms most commonly met with average about o.i mm. in length and 0.015 mm. in diameter. Nearly all are green, having numerous chloro- OBSERVATIONS ON UNICELLULAR FORMS 8l plasts of various forms. They have a contractile vacuole which opens to the exterior at the anterior end, and a brown pigment spot known as the eye-spot, in close connection with the vacuole. They exist m three states, — free-swimming, crawling and encysted (Fig. lo). In the free-swimming C 'p F -0.01 mm. Fig. io. Sketches of Euglena, showing general structure of dififerent forms. A and C, Euglena :*; sp. (?) in crawling state; B, probably a form of E. viridis; D, E, E. deses; e, eye-spot; v, contractile vacuole; ch, chloroplasts; space in B limited by dotted lines well filled with small chloroplasts; n, nucleus; c, caudal spine; p, pigment granules which appear to be composed of same substances as eye-spot, — these were found in only a few specimens. E, shows typical curvature toward dorsal surface while swimming in direction indicated by arrow. F, eye-spot highly magnified; s, surface view; a, view from anterior end. The convex surface is directed outward, mm., projected scale. All outlines were made with camera from specimens killed in iodine. Contractile vacuoles and nuclei were sketched free-hand from Uving specimens. state they have a flagellum frequently nearly as long as the body. Wager (1900) found that in E. viridis it passes down through the opening of the contractile vacuole and divides into two branches, each of which is attached to the wall of the vacuole. One of these branches contains an enlargement which lies directly opposite the eye-spot, as ^2 LIGHT AND TEE BEHAVIOR OF ORGANISMS represented in Fig. ii. Under certain conditions some forms cast off the flagellum, sink to the bottom and crawl about in a manner to be described in detail later. In the encysted state, as is well known, they are inactive. h. Historical account. — It has long been known that these organisms in their free-swimming state orient and -c.v. swim toward a source of light, and Stahl (1880, p. 410) found that if the light intensity is high they become negative, i.e., they swim away from the source of light. Engelmann (1882, Fig. II. Side view of p. 396) observed that if Euglenae are anterior end of Euglena ,1 1 • j ^ • ' ±. viridis, after Wager;., eye- Hiountcd on a slide contammg a spot spot; /, flagellum; e./., en- of relatively Strong light they collect largement in flagellum; c.r., • j • . i • . • , contractile vacuole. ^"^ dense masses m^ this spot just as Paramecia collect in regions contain- ing a little CO2. They swim into it without any ap- parent reaction, but when they reach the boundary on the way out they stop suddenly, turn around, and thus remain in the illuminated area. Engelmann called this reaction Schreckhewegung, shock-movement, and Jennings, avoiding reaction. Engelmann also proved that the ante- rior end of E. viridis is more sensitive than the posterior. Jennings (1904) however was the first to demonstrate the connection between the shock-movement, the sudden turning when subjected to a decrease in illumination, and orientation, although the idea expressed in the fol- lowing words shows that Engelmann (1882, p. 395) was also very near the truth in this matter: " Falls sie, was bei schnellem Vorwartsschwimmen wohl einmal geschieht, gans ins Dunkel hineingekommen sind, sistiren sie doch so fort die weitere Vorwartsbewegung, drehen um eine ihrer kurzen Axen, probiren — oft unter bedeutenden Gestaltsanderungen — in verschiedenen Richtungen fortzu- kommen bis sie endlich wieder ins Licht gerathen." OBSERVATIONS ON UNICELLULAR FORMS 83 Jennings found that as Euglena swims on its spiral course it rotates on its long axis so as to keep the side containing the eye-spot constantly facing out, and that when it is stimulated it always turns toward this side, which is desig- nated the dorsal side. The process of orientation is de- scribed as follows (1906, p. 138): "The Euglenae are swimming about at random in a diffuse light, when a stronger light is allowed to fall upon them from one side. Thereupon the forward movement becomes slower and the Euglenae begin to swerve farther than usual toward the dorsal side. Thus the spiral path becomes wider and the anterior end swings about in a larger circle and is pointed successively in many different directions. In some part of its swinging in a circle the anterior end of course be- comes directed more nearly toward the light ; thereupon the amount of swinging decreases, so that the Euglena tends to retain a certain position so reached. In other parts of the swinging in a circle the anterior end becomes less exposed to the light; thereupon the swaying increases, so that the organism does not retain this position, but swings to another. The result is that in its spiral course it suc- cessively swerves strongly toward the source of light, then slightly away from it, until by a continuation of this process the anterior end is directed toward the light. In this position it swims forward. The course of Euglena in becoming oriented is shown in " Fig. 12. Orientation in Euglena is, therefore, according to Jen- nings, indirect. The stimulus resulting in orientation is due to changes in light intensity on the organism. The direction of the rays functions in orientation only in so far as it makes such changes possible. Changes of intensity on the organism may be due to movement from a region of one intensity to that of another, or to a change in the axial position of the organism with reference to the source of light. There Is no evidence that orientation is due to a constantly acting directive stimulus In accord with Loeb's theory of troplsms. Jennings does not deny that the 84 LIGHT AND THE BEHAVIOR OF ORGANISMS Euglenae are affected by light after they are oriented. He thinks, however, that whatever such effects may be, they are relatively unimportant in the process of orientation. ^— f ^\-~.e_,^^"~^\ b{ }a 4^ L 4 ^^_ Fig. 12. Illustration of the devious path followed by Euglena in becoming oriented when the direction of the light is reversed. From i to 2 the light comes from above; at 2 it is reversed. The amount of wandering {a-h) varies in different cases. After Jennings (1906, p. 137). Torrey (1907, pp. 317, 319) criticizes the analysis pre- sented by Jennings in the following terms: " My analysis of their responses, based upon the figure which Jennings him- OBSERVATIONS ON UNICELLULAR FORMS 85 self has drawn, with text description, leads to quite a different conclusion from his. The figure indicates that Euglena is both unterschiedsempfindlich and heliotropic. At a (Fig. 12) the reversal in the direction of the light, which has been coming from the direction in which the creature has been swimming, produces a sudden change in intensity of stimulation, a shock which results in the swerv- ing from the previous course, as indicated between a and c. The organism recovers rapidly, only to be subjected to the constant stimulus of a steady light from one direction to the end of the experiment. The result of the action of the constant stimulus is a path, from c to 5, so perfectly in harmony with the tropic schema, that, in spite of Jennings' descriptions and elucidations, I can only wonder at his running so boldly and so far into the enemy's camp. . . . In heliotropism . . . the oriented organism is in a condi- tion of physiological stimulation, and . . . the response to stimulation is local ; finally, . . . the interpretation of the behavior of heliotropic organisms on the basis of general changes concerning the whole organism, not only does not accord with the main facts, but is rather psychical than physiological in character." It is thus evident that while Torrey recognizes that Euglena responds to change of light intensity, he considers that orientation is due to the local effect of unequal stimu- lation of symmetrically situated points on the body, and that after the organism is oriented it is held upon its course by constantly acting directive stimulation. He does not, however, explain where the symmetrically located points which are subject to local stimulation are situated in Euglena. They might be conceived to be in the flagellum or in the body. In the former case It would imply direct action of the point stimulated, In the latter a reaction in harmony with the location of the stimulus, i.e., if the stimulus is applied to the left side of the body the flagellum would strike toward the left; if applied to the right side. It would strike toward the right, etc. 86 LIGHT AND THE BEHAVIOR OF ORGANISMS If Euglenae actually orient by local response to local stimulation, as Torrey assumes, or if light acts constantly as a directive stimulus in accord with Loeb's theory, one should be able to find evidence of it in these organisms in the crawling state. With this in mind, therefore, I took up the study of specimens in this state. Before entering on the description of the reactions in Euglena bearing directly on the problem just stated, I shall however refer briefly to the question of orientation in light from several sources, since the experimental results obtained under these conditions throw some light on the idea of Sachs, that the direction of the rays through the organism regulates orientation, and on Loeb's idea that symmetrically located points on the sensitive surface must be struck by rays at the same angle when an organism is oriented. c. Orientation in light from two sources. — In studying the movement of Euglenae in light from two sources, Nernst glowers in a dark room were so arranged and screened as to produce two small horizontal beams of light which crossed each other at right angles in the aquarium. One glower was stationary. The other was mounted on a horizontal track so that it could easily be pushed nearer to or farther away from the aquarium. Thus the relative intensity from the two glowers could be changed without any change in the direction of the rays. Several species of Euglena in the free swimming state, and two, Euglena deses and Euglena x in the crawling state, were used in these experiments. The results were the same in all. When the light from the two glowers was equal and the Euglenae positive they moved in a general way toward a point very nearly halfway between the glowers. But when it was unequal, they moved toward a point nearer the source from which the more intense light came. Negative specimens take the same general course but in the opposite direction. This experiment is particularly striking if the glower on the track is gradually moved from a position in OBSERVATIONS ON UNICELLULAR FORMS 87 which the Hght intensity from it is much lower than that from the stationary glower to a position in which it is much higher. Under such conditions one can clearly see these organisms, especially the free-swimming forms, gradually change their direction of motion through an angle of nearly 90°. (Just how this change is brought about will be demonstrated later.) By regulating the relative intensity of the light from the two sources, it is thus possible to cause Euglenae to move toward any point between the two sources of light without changing the direction of the rays. It is evident then that the direction of the rays does not absolutely control the direction of motion. These results are in harmony with those which I obtained in experiments on Volvox (1907, p. 134). Identical results were also obtained in light from two sources with Stentor coeruleus, Trachelo- monas, Chlamydomonas, Oedogonium swarm-spores, Eu- dorina, Pandorina, Planulae of Eudendrium, Limulus polyphemus larvae, Musca larvae, Allolobophora foetida, medusae of Bougainvillea superciliaris, trochophores of Hydroides dianthus, Arenicola larvae, zoeae, several forms, and Leptoplana tremellaris. Judging from these results it is highly probable that all individuals without image- forming eyes orient in the same way under like conditions. All of these forms can be induced to change their direc- tion of motion by varying the relative light intensity on opposite sides of the body, or by changing the intensity on the same side, without changing the direction of the rays. It may therefore be concluded that difference in the inten- sity of light on opposite sides, or a change of intensity on the same side of the body of all these creatures, may deter- mine orientation independently of the direction of the rays. The orientation of organisms without image-form- ing eyes can therefore not be explained by the application of Sachs' ray direction theory, nor are the orienting reac- tions in harmony with the statements of Loeb expressed in the following quotations: (1905, p. 2), " It is explicitly stated in this and the following papers that if there are 88 LIGHT AND THE BEHAVIOR OF ORGANISMS several sources of light of unequal intensity, the light with the strongest intensity determines the orientation and direction of motion of the animal. Other possible compli- cations are covered by the unequivocal statement, made and emphasized in this and the following papers on the same subject, that the main feature in all phenomena of heliotropism is the fact that symmetrical points of the photosensitive surface of the animal must be struck by the rays of light at the same angle. It is in full harmony with this fact that if two sources of light of equal intensity and distance act simultaneously upon a heliotropic animal, the animal puts its median plane at right angles to the line connecting the two sources of light. This fact was not only known to me but had been demonstrated by me on the larvae of flies as early as 1887, in Wiirzburg, and often enough since. These facts seem to have escaped several of my critics; " (p. 61), " When the diffuse daylight which struck the [Musca] larvae came from two window^s, the planes of which were at an angle of 90° with each other, the paths taken by the larvae lay diagonally between the two planes. . . . This experiment was recently published by an American physiologist as a new discovery to prove that I had overlooked the importance of the intensity of light!" (p. 82), '' The direction of the median plane or the direction of the progressive movements of an animal coincides with the direction of the rays of light ... if there is only a single source of light. If there are two sources of light of different intensities, the animal is oriented by the stronger of the two lights. If their intensities be equal, the animal is oriented in such a w^ay as to have symmetrical points of its body struck by the rays at the same angle;" (p. 268), " Attention need scarcely be called to the fact that if rays of light strike the animal [larvae of Limulus polyphemus] simultaneously from various directions, and the animal is able to move freely in all directions, the more intense rays will determine the direction of the progressive movements." Note that this animal is in the list mentioned above (p. 87). OBSERVATIONS ON UNICELLULAR FORMS 89 Under the conditions of the experiment described above, the organisms mentioned do not move in a direction parallel with the rays, neither do they necessarily orient so " that symmetrical points of the photosensitive surface [are] struck by the rays of light at the same angle," nor does " the light with the strongest intensity determine the orientation and direction of motion." Toads (Bufo americanus) were the only animals w^ith image -forming eyes that were tested with reference to orientation in light from two sources (see p. 87). If the intensity from the two sources is unequal they usually hop directly toward the stronger light and pay no attention to the weaker. This is in accord with Loeb's explanation given above. But if the intensity from the two sources is equal, they go toward either one and not toward a point between the two, as Loeb's explanation demands. In none of the organisms studied are the orienting reactions such as are demanded by Loeb's explanation. These results will be referred to in connection with the discussion of the importance of equal stimulation of symmetrical points on the animal. Let us now return to our study of the reactions of Euglena in the crawling state and to the problem suggested by Torrey's criticism of Jennings referred to above. Is orien- tation in Euglena due to light acting constantly as a direc- tive stimulation similar to the effect of a constant electric current, or to an intermittent effect, a response to change of intensity only, in accord with Jennings' explanation? d. Material. — During the months of November and December excellent material for this study was discovered in a puddle of water fed by a drain from a dwelling house at Windsor Hills, Baltimore. The bottom of the puddle was covered with a dense green layer composed almost entirely of two species of Euglena, — E. deses and another species which was somewhat like viridis but could not be positively identified. It will be referred to as Euglena x. Most of the E. deses had fiagella, but the E. x with very QO LIGHT AND THE BEHAVIOR OF ORGANISMS few exceptions had none. The latter were considerably smaller than the former. They averaged nearly 0.08 mm. in length and somewhat more than 0.015 mm. in diameter. A fairly good idea of the form and structure may be obtained by referring to Fig. 10. It will be seen in this figure that the caudal end terminates in a spinelike process, and that the eye-spot, in close contact with the canal leading from the contractile vacuole, forms an angle of about 45° with the long axis of the body. The eye-spot has the form of a flattened disk somewhat curved, so as to fit around the canal. e. Method of locomotion. — It is frequently stated that Euglenae in this state progress by amoeboid movements, i.e., by streaming movements. I was, however, unable to detect anything resembling streaming movements in any of the several different species studied in the crawling state. Many do change their form very much by contracting in various ways, and some may move slightly by thrusting the anterior end forward and then drawing up the posterior end, but progression in this way is relatively unimportant. The process of locomotion without flagella appears to be much the same in all forms observed. It was however studied in detail only in Euglena x. While in motion these organisms usually are considerably curved, being convex on the ventral surface, the side opposite the eye-spot. They rotate on the long axis either entirely over to the left, as seen from the posterior end, or only halfway, then back again, lying on the dorsal surface during this apparent rocking movement. During either of these rotating move- ments both ends appear to move back and forth. The posterior end however moves laterally much less than the anterior. In many instances it continues forward in nearly a straight path, while the anterior end progresses on a spiral course of considerable relative width. While thus rotating the organisms appear to slide along, moving forward a little with each turn of the body. They progress at the rate of about 0.3 mm. per minute. Pre- OBSERVATIONS ON UNICELLULAR FORMS 91 cisely what factors are involved in causing the forward movement I was not able to ascertain. Only very slight contractions can be seen at any time and no streaming movements at all. The posterior end is in much closer contact with the sub- stratum than the anterior. If currents of water are passed back and forth over the Euglenae it can be seen that the anterior end is free, for it moves with the current. Fre- quently specimens are found attached to the slide with only the tip of the caudal spine in contact with the sur- face. In such specimens the whole body sw^ings about with the current. They are held fast by an adhesive substance which they secrete. The presence of such a substance can be detected by passing a small glass rod across the path of a crawling individual near its posterior end, or by pushing the rod about on a slide containing numerous Euglenae which have been crawling about for a short time. If this is done the end of the rod soon becomes covered with a substance to which cling numerous Euglenae attached usually only at the posterior end. It is however not likely that the extrusion of the secretion forces the Euglenae along, as is supposed to be true in the case of diatoms. The body appears to become alternately more and less curved as they rotate in such a way as to force them forward. The caudal spine appears to be used as a sort of lever in this movement. They can however move without the use of the spine, for moving specimens were repeatedly seen in which the point of the spine was not in contact with the slide at all. This was evident especially in specimens which rotated only partially over and then back again. As these creatures crawl along, rotating on the long axis with the anterior end progressing on a spiral course, the dorsal surface, the surface containing the eye-spot, always faces the axis of the spiral. This is just the opposite of Jennings' observations on Euglena viridis in the free- swimming state. I found however that E. acus and a few other species swim with the dorsal side facing the axis 92 LIGHT AND THE BEHAVIOR OF ORGANISMS of the spiral and that E. deses swims with one side facing the axis. Euglenae in the crawHng state, just as in the free-swim- ming state, may be either negative or positive in their light reactions. The crawhng specimens worked on were however negative to Hght of surprisingly low intensity throughout the entire work. But very few were found which were positive even in diffuse sunlight during the middle of the day, unless the sky was covered with very dense clouds. The cause of reversal in the sense of orien- tation will be discussed elsewhere. /. Accuracy of orientation. — In the study of their reactions to light, the Euglenae w^ere exposed either to sunlight direct and diffused, or to light from a Nernst glower, a Welsbach burner or a carbon filament. When exposed to light from a single source, e.g., a Nernst glower so arranged that there is as little reflection as possible, Euglenae orient and move nearly straight toward or away from the light with little deviation, if they are strongly positive or negative; but if they are not, as is frequently the case, they deviate much. Even under the most favorable conditions there is however little similarity between Eugle- nae moving toward a source of light and iron filings moving toward a magnet, a comparison sometimes met with in the literature on reactions to light. In studying Euglenae one always finds specimens which do the unexpected thing. Their reactions are very much less dependent upon external conditions than are the reactions of iron filings. To come to a full realization of this, one need only consider the fact that these organisms may be negative or positive in almost any light intensity or they may not react at all. To predict with any degree of accuracy what these organisms are going to do under given conditions, it is necessary to know much about the history of their past reactions. g. Mechanics of orientation in Euglena x in the crawling state. — Nernst glowers mounted in front of a non-reflect- ing background (see Fig. 4) and properly screened in a large OBSERVATIONS ON UNICELLULAR FORMS 93 dark room were used in all quantitative work, and in all work in which it was desirable to regulate the direction of the rays. I have elsewhere pointed out the advan- tageous features of these glowers for such work (1906, P- 363)- The general movements of Euglenae could readily be followed under a Braus-Driiner binocular, but it was found necessary to use a compound microscope in working out the details in the reactions owing to the small size of the organisms. They progress so slowly however that every movement can easily be followed even under the highest magnification. They are consequently very favorable for the work in hand notwithstanding their minute size. In studying the process of orientation the microscope was placed either in front of two windows in the laboratory so situated that the general direction of the light entering them was at right angles on the stage, or in the dark room in a similar relative position in front of two Nernst glowers (see Fig. 13). The two glowers were mounted so that the rays were practically parallel with the plane of the stage. One was stationary; the other was mounted on a track so that the distance between it and the aquarium could readily be varied, and thus the intensity of the light from it on the stage changed without any change in the direction of the rays. Both glowers were of the same kind and both were in the same circuit, so that any fluctuation in the current affected both alike. The relation in light intensity from the two sources could thus be regulated as desired. The glowers were so screened that only a small beam from each reached the stage, and this could readily be cut off from either or both. The Euglenae were either mounted on a slide under a cover-glass or exposed in a rectangular glass aquarium made for the purpose by cementing slides together with balsam and linseed oil. After they had oriented in light from one of the two sources, the light from that source was cut off and that from the other turned on simultaneously. In 94 LIGHT AND THE BEHAVIOR OP ORGANISMS this way their reactions during the process of reorientation could be studied in detail. The following description of this process refers to E. x in the crawling state. If the light in which positive organisms are oriented is decreased in intensity without a change in the direction of the rays, e.g., by pushing back the Nernst glower on the track, they respond in a characteristically definite way. If the decrease is relatively slight the anterior end is merely turned toward the ventral surface, the whole body becomes more curved and the spiral course of the anterior end becomes wider. If however the decrease is considerable, they frequently stop in their forward motion and turn the anterior end toward the ventral surface to such an extent that the two halves of the organism form a right angle. In this condition they continue to rotate, turning over and over in the same spot, and appear to be squirming and twisting about aimlessly. They soon however straighten again and continue on their way toward the source of light, having apparently become acclimatized to the change in light intensity. If the intensity is increased there is no response in positive Euglenae. Negative individuals, on the contrary, respond precisely as described above if the light intensity is increased, but not at all if it is decreased. If the specimens however are only slightly positive or nega- tive they may be caused to respond with this twisting reac- tion either by increasing the intensity or by decreasing it. In order to induce this reaction it is necessary to change the intensity at a certain rate. If the glower is moved back very slowly and steadily, no reaction whatever is seen. A sudden decrease of intensity then without any change in the direction of the rays produces a definite reaction in positive individuals, and a sudden increase of intensity produces the same reaction in negative individuals. These reactions are in accord with the shock effects of Engelmann and Pfeffer and Unlerschiedsempfindlichkeit of Loeb. They are not due to an absolute change of intensity but to the time rate of change of intensity. The amount of change OBSERVATIONS ON UNICELLULAR FORMS 95 necessary to induce a reaction will be discussed later (p. 105). If the intensity from the two sources of light arranged as described above is equal and the beams which reach the stage of the microscope are alternately cut off with an opaque screen so as to change the direction of the rays suddenly without changing the intensity, it appears as though the Euglenae if positive always turn directly toward the source of light, never away from it no matter in what position they are or which surface becomes illuminated when the ray direction is changed. These results would seem to indicate that there is here a local response to a local stimulation, or at least differential response to local- ized stimulation. I was firmly convinced of the truth of this for several days, as were also other members of the laboratory who observed these reactions. Further work however demonstrated the fallacy of this conclusion. By very careful observations under the high power it was found that if the ventral surface, the surface opposite the eye-spot, faces the source of light, after the direction of the rays is changed, there is no immediate reaction. The Euglenae continue on their course as though no change had taken place until the rotation on the long axis carries the dorsal surface over into a position in which it faces the light. As soon as this surface, the surface containing the eye-spot, faces the light there is a definite reaction. The Euglenae turn the anterior end toward the ventral surface more or less sharply, i.e., away from the source of light, but they continue to rotate so that the ventral surface soon faces the light again; but it is evident, owing to the curva- ture in the body, that the anterior end is now directed more nearly toward its source than it was when this surface faced the light during the preceding rotation. While in this position, the body is somewhat straightened, so that the anterior end is not carried back as far during the following rotation, and when the dorsal surface comes to face the light it is directed more nearly toward its source than it 96 LIGHT AND THE BEHAVIOR OF ORGANISMS was when the organism was in this position before, as represented in Fig. 13. This reaction is repeated during each complete rotation. Every time the eye-spot becomes $ ^ I * t ». '■' i'/ i/ <' o •<= ^« -«^ -^< -^0< -^i^^ -^S -«a -<^ ■