A METHOD OF ASSESSING JUNGIAN PSYCHOLOGICAL TYPE DEVELOPMENT IN A HIGH SCHOOL STUDENT SAMPLE BY JANIE DARLENE SWEET A DISSERTATION PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE COUNCIL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA 1981 Copyright 1981 by Janie Darlene Sweet ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS Words cannot express adequately my appreciation and heart- felt thanks to my committee, Richard Anderson, Wilson Guertin, and especially to my chairman, Donald Avila, for putting up with me all these years, for too many phone calls during meetings, and for my showing up on trips from South Carolina to Florida with very little notice. Don has served me well as an advisor and was always there when I needed someone to listen and be a friend. To Drs . Margaret K. Morgan and Mary H. McCaulley, my love and appreciation for long years of support, encourage- ment, and guidance. My feelings for them are equally shared by my husband, George. Thanks to the staff of the Center for Applications of Psychological Type for their assistance and patience in helping me get ready to conduct the testing phase of the study. To Richard Kainz , doctoral student in clinical psy- chology and long-time member of the CAPT staff, I am indebted in a multitude of ways-- for his part in data collection, for fearlessly helping me tackle the computer, for playing devil's advocate, and for being a true friend. To Claudette Connolly, my dearest and closest friend, there is not room to express my appreciation for all the years of encouragement and support. My only regret that this dissertation is finished is in no longer having a valid excuse to visit you often. To Dr. Drew Barrett, Assistant Principal at Sarasota High School at the time this study was conducted, my deepest and warmest thanks to you and to the kids, for making the study possible and for all your valuable efforts and time in providing the needed information. My thanks to my family, George, Scott, and George, Jr. , for hanging in there during the disappointments and the sacrifices, and for sharing in the joys. Anything that I have accomplished would not have been possible without their patience, understanding, and love. Thanks to my parents, my aunt, and my grandmother for their love, support, and all the babysitting so I could work knowing my children were in good hands. To the spirit of Isabel Briggs Myers--I regret I could not finish this in time for her reading- -her work and her being have inspired so many. This is dedicated in memorium to her. CABLE OF CONTENTS PAGE ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS iii LIST OF TABLES vii LIST OF FIGURES ix ABSTRACT x CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION 1 Purpose of the Study 1 Statement of the Problem 1 Theoretical Basis for the Study 4 Focus of the Present Study 7 Hypotheses 9 II RELEVANT LITERATURE 10 Jung's Theory of Psychological Types 10 The Developmental Nature of the Theory. . 10 Extraversion, Introversion and the Four Psychological Functions 14 The Origin of Psychological Type Preferences 17 An Interpretation of the Dominant and the Auxiliary Function 25 Evidence Related to the Value of Clarity and Strength of Psychological Type Preferences 30 An Assessment Problem 44 III DESIGN OF THE STUDY 47 Subjects 47 Procedure 49 Instrumentation Employed in the Study 51 The Myers -Briggs Type Indicator 51 Description 51 Validity 52 Reliability 55 CHAPTER TABLE OF CONTENTS (Continued) PAGE The Moral Judgment Questionnaire 58 Description 58 Validity 59 Reliability 62 Demographic and School Performance Data 63 Demographic 63 School Performance 63 The Discrepancy Index 64 Statement of Specific Hypotheses 68 Data Analysis 70 IV RESULTS 73 One-way Analysis of Variance 73 Post Hoc Comparisons 75 Selection Ratio Type Table Analyses 75 Frequencies 76 Results of Hypotheses Testing 76 V DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSIONS 85 Discussion of Results 85 Summary 93 Implications for Education 94 Limitations and Suggestions for Further Research 98 APPENDICES A SUPPLEMENTARY ANALYSES OF TEACHER ESTIMATES OF READING LEVEL 103 B DESCRIPTIVE STATISTICS FOR DISCREPANCY INDICES 105 C TYPE DISTRIBUTION IN STUDY SAMPLE Ill D RESULTS AND DISCUSSION OF SELECTION RATIO TYPE TABLE (SRTT) ANALYSES 117 BIBLIOGRAPHY 124 BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH 131 LIST OF TABLES TABLE PAGE 1 Subjects broken down by grade level 48 2 Confidence ranges for reported preferences 65 3 Continuous score ranges differing significantly from chance 66 4 Significant findings for H 77 2 1 5 Significant findings for H 79 6 Significant findings for H2-2 and H2 ' 4 81 7 Significant findings for H 84 APPENDICES Al Significant findings for teacher estimates of reading level 104 Bl Descriptive statistics for DI's reflecting strength of preference 106 B2 Descriptive statistics for DI's reflecting word-pair/phrase discrepancies 107 B3 Descriptive statistics for DI's reflecting split-half differences 108 B4 Descriptive statistics for total discrepancies for each MBTI scale 109 B5 Descriptive statistics for combined discrepancies on all 4 MBTI scales 110 CI Psychological type distribution for 485 high school students 114 C2 Myers' students in Pennsylvania high schools 115 C3 Comparison of 485 high school students with Myers' Pennsylvania high school student sample... 116 vii LIST OF TABLES (Continued) TABLE PAGE Dl SRTT analysis for D1EI groups 1 and 3 121 D2 SRTT analysis for D1TF groups 2 and 4 122 D3 SRTT analysis for D4SN groups 1 and 3 123 LIST OF FIGURES FIGURE PAGE 1 Diagram illustrating the basic psychological type preferences 29 2 Myers' schema for determining dominant function 30 Abstract of Dissertation Presented to the Graduate Council of the University of Florida in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy A METHOD OF ASSESSING JUNGIAN PSYCHOLOGICAL TYPE DEVELOPMENT IN A HIGH SCHOOL STUDENT SAMPLE By JANIE DARLENE SWEET August 1981 Chairman: Donald Avila Major Department: Foundations of Education A Discrepancy Index designed to indicate the level of psychological type development at which a person is func- tioning was constructed and evaluated. The index was derived from the following three categories of discrepant or inconsistent responses to the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI) : (1) when a person has a very low overall score in general for a particular scale of the MBTI; (2) a person gives answers favoring one pole of a scale on the word-pair items and then chooses the opposite pole on the phrase questions, or vice versa; and/or (3) a person gives answers favoring one pole or an MBTI scale on the x or y-half of the MBTI and then favors the opposite pole on the other half. The focus of the study was to determine, in a large sample of metropolitan Florida high school students (N = 485) , whether the Discrepancy Index has the capability to differentiate between students high and low in level of moral reasoning and high and low on academic variables (overall and academic grade point average and class rank) . It appears that the Discrepancy Index dealing with strength of preference has discriminating power. As pre- dicted, students expressing high preference for their per- ceptive function, whether it is for Sensing or for Intuition, fared better academically than students with low preferences for their perceptive function. A similar rela- tionship existed for high Moral Judgment Scale score and high preference for their judging function, either Thinking or Feeling. The Discrepancy Indices for word-pair/phrase discrepancy and x-y half differences need further adjustment and re- evaluation. As constructed for the present study they predicted very little. More sensitive dependent measures than grade point average should be utilized. Results of the study do provide evidence that assessment of "within type" differences is reasonable and possible, and that strong, clear preference appears to be an important and measurable component of good type development. The results obtained provide further support for the theoretical assumption that it is the clarity and degree of development of a preference, and not which preference is favored, that accounts for competence in perception and judgment. xi CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION Purpose of the Study The purpose of this study was to evaluate the validity of a Discrepancy Index designed to indicate the level of psychological type development at which a person is func- tioning. The Discrepancy Index was constructed from three categories of discrepant or inconsistent responses to the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI) . Statement of the Problem Some of the problems facing the public school system have reached or are reaching a crisis point. The push for accountability in schools is gaining momentum. When large proportions of students cannot pass functional literacy tests it is no surprise that the schools bear the burden of blame. Part of the total problem is that there is no con- sensus among members of the American public as to what the schools should, in fact, be teaching. Once goals are clearly delineated by school administrators and curriculum planners, the important question then becomes how to teach that which is supposed to be taught (a matter of methods rather than of goals) . One method which has surfaced is the attempt to individualize the educational process. A problem 2 here is to find ways to individualize instruction while achieving cost efficiency. The successful individualized approach will be one which provides a straightforward and inexpensive way to discover and deal with individual differ- ences of students in their own approaches to learning situa- tions. A great deal of research on individualizing instruction is taking place all over the country. It is this researcher's belief that if we cannot very soon, and on a large scale, satisfactorily implement in the classroom the fruits of these researches, the movement to individualize will go the route of so many other short-lived educational innovations . Carl G. Jung (1921) proposed a theory of individual differences which is currently being examined and evaluated in educational settings as a means of individualizing instruction. Research has clearly established that people do differ in the basic preferences for the use of the psy- chological functions and attitudes proposed by Jung, and that these preferences make a difference in motivation, aptitude and achievement. In addition, students differ in the command that they have over their preferred functions. Teachers and counselors dealing with psychological type concepts in the classroom have made similar observations. In describing the psychological type preferences Jung (1921) , Myers (1962) and others (McCaulley, 1978; Quenk, 1978; Van der Hoop, 1939; von Franz & Hillman, 1971) comment upon dif- ferences between persons with excellent type development and those 3 with problems in type development. In other words, it is expected that some individuals will have developed their preferred process or function to a greater extent than others preferring use of the same function. The degree to which students have developed their preferred functions has implications for how well they perform in day-to-day situa- tions which call upon their powers of perception and their powers of judgment. In this respect, the present study is at a far more individual and micro- level than that of dealing with basic type differences. The focus is on individual differences within types as much as with the differences between types which have been the concern of earlier research. Knowledge of psychological type preferences of students is beneficial to educators and to students. In elementary level classrooms, however, it may not be necessary or even advisable to determine the type preferences of particular students. At this age level students are gaining experience and expertise in their own approaches to learning situations. Many elementary level teachers have found it useful, however, to estimate type through observation (Nuernberger & Lawrence, 1974) . Knowledge of activities that appeal to students with different psychological type preferences allows the instruc- tor to offer a "smorgaasbord"--where, indeed, there is something for everybody and students can gain experiences which develop all their powers of perception and judgment. 4 By the time a student reaches the high school level, an indication of the type preferences of the individual becomes relevant for more than understanding communication between teachers and students. It becomes relevant for decisions about lifelong goals, including career decisions. It is particularly important for students experiencing academic difficulty, and for their teachers, to assess not only their type preferences but to determine whether or not they really have command over the psychological functions they prefer to use. It is for these students that the educational dollar to individualize instruction can have the highest return. Public Law 94-142 has mandated the development of highly individualized educational programs for all handicapped learners. Learning disabled students are a target group for highly individualized instruction. Use of the MBIT to assess type preferences of learning disabled students is being explored (Metts, 1979). At this point in time we can assess the basic type preferences with a considerable degree of reliability. The pressing problem is to discover a reliable means to appraise "the potent but as yet unmeasurable variable of 'type development ' --i. e. , the extent to which the person actually has developed the processes and the attitudes which he pre- fers" (Myers, 1962, p. 19). Theoretical Basis for the Study The approach to individualizing instruction being utilized in the present study is based on Carl G. Jung's 5 theory of psychological types (1921) . The theory is highly relevant to education because it focuses on the individual's conscious use of his/her powers of perception and his/her powers of judgment. The theory begins at the point where the students focus their attention to take in information or know something (perceive) and then to decide or come to con- clusions (judge) about it. Some students will naturally be better at perceiving than judging and vice versa, but all individuals need to develop their full potential for both processes . An instrument, the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI) , designed to implement Jung's theory of psychological types, was developed by Isabel Myers in the 1940' s. After careful study and evaluation, the MBTI was published as a research instrument by Educational Testing Service in 1962 and as a tool for general professional use by Consulting Psycholo- gists Press in 1975. A substantial body of research using this instrument in education is available (Bibliography, Center for Applications of Psychological Type, 1981). Psychological type differences have been fruitfully studied. Attempts to individualize instruction based on these findings are meeting with success. Gifted students and others with special talents have been studied from the perspective of psychological type differences. Increased knowledge of student preferences for educational media based on type differences is emerging (Anast, 1966; Golanty-Koel , 1977; Williams, 1973). A substantial proportion of the literature on psychological type differences in students deals with academic aptitude and performance. With so much current interest in cognitive structures, perceptual organi- zation, and teaching and learning "styles" it is not sur- prising to find a large proportion of studies in those areas taking psychological type variables into account in settings that range from middle school through graduate and profes- sional training. Research over more than three decades has established the reliability of the MBIT (Carlyn, 1977; Coan, 1978; Myers, 1962) and the practical utility of Jung's theory in the educational process (Neurnberger & Lawrence, 1974; McCaulley, 1974, 1976a, 1976b; von Franz & Hillman, 1971; Wickes, 1966) . Myers (1980) considers a major goal of education to be the fostering of psychological type development in order to improve and enhance learning and to prevent or head off future problems. In this writer's opinion we have reached a major roadblock in accomplishing this goal, and an obstacle for psychological type research in the educational process in general. We have looked extensively at basic type differences between students in all sorts of fields, programs and settings. What we do not yet know is the amount of variance in our studies that might potentially be accounted for if we could identify persons who are at a high level of type development (regardless of which preferences they like and use best) from those who are less differenti- ated and at low 1 eve 1 s of type development. 7 Before we can study this issue further, we must have a way to measure type development. Research related to education employing the MBTI has not yet dealt centrally or systematically with the issue of individual differences in type development. There are several clues that such studies will be productive. Myers (1962) found underachieving stu- dents in her sample to have lower internal consistency measures on the MBTI than high achievers. She also found aptitude and achievement scores for high school and college students in her sample to be lower for those potentially less clear about their type preferences. Focus of the Present Study In day-to-day situations, inconsistent or discrepant responses to an MBTI scale are being interpreted as suggestive of inadequate development of the psychological process that the scale measures. Before any lack of development can be assumed to exist, more direct evidence regarding the meaning of inconsistent response patterns must be established. It was the focus of the present study to construct and evaluate one possible index of level of psychological type development. The focus was three- fold. First, a Dis- crepancy Index was created which operationalizes the con- cepts of inconsistent response and strength of type preference. The inconsistencies or discrepancies upon which the Discrepancy Index was built are: (1) when a person responds or gives answers favoring one pole of an MBTI scale on the X or Y-half of the MBTI and then favors the opposite pole on the other half; (2) a person gives answers favoring one pole of a scale on the word-pair items of the MBTI and then chooses the opposite pole on the phrase-question items, or vice versa; and/or (3) a person has a very low overall score in general for a particular scale of the MBTI. Secondly, it was determined whether, in a large sample of high school students (N = 485) , a relationship exists between discrepant responding on the MBTI as measured by the Discrepancy Index and students' responses to a Moral Judg- ment Questionnaire. The Moral Judgment Scale was developed by Maitland and Goldman (1974) to assess the level of develop- ment of moral reasoning (judgment) that a person employs. Finally, the academic and overall gradepoint average and class rank of students with low Discrepancy Index scores were compared with those of students with high Discrepancy Index scores to determine whether a relationship exists between discrepant response pattern and measures of academic performance . It was also expected that students with a high Dis- crepancy Index score on the Sensing-Intuition (S-N) scale of the MBTI would have lower reading levels than students with low Discrepancy Index scores on the S-N scale. Reading test scores could be made available from school records for so few students that this relationship was not tested formally in the present study. English teachers did provide the investigator with estimates of students' reading level. 9 Supplemental analyses of these estimates of reading level with Discrepancy Index scores were performed and are reported in Appendix A. Hypotheses H Students who respond in a consistent and non- discrepant pattern on the MBIT will employ a higher level of moral reasoning than students who respond in an inconsistent and discrepant pattern on the MBTI. 2 H Students who respond in a consistent and non-discrepant pattern on the MBTI will have demonstrated higher academic performance than students who respond in an inconsistent and discrepant pattern on the MBTI. 3 H Students with low Discrepancy Index scores on the Thinking-Feeling scale of the MBTI when it represents their theorized Dominant Function will employ a higher level of moral reasoning than students with high Discrepancy Index scores on the T-F scale when it represents their theorized Dominant Function. 4 H Students with low Discrepancy Index scores on the Sensing- Intuition scale of the MBTI when it represents their theorized Dominant Function will have attained a higher academic and overall grade point average, or higher academic and overall class rank than students with high Discrepancy Index scores on the S-N scale when it represents their theorized Dominant Function. CHAPTER II RELEVANT LITERATURE In the first portion of this chapter the basic concepts of Jung's theory of psychological types are reviewed and the developmental aspects of the theory explicated. In part two research and clinical observations related to the value of an individual's holding clear and consistent psychological type preferences are examined. Part three delineates an assessment problem in psychological type research. Jung's Theory of Psychological Types The Developmental Nature of the Theory Increasing recognition of individual differences and the need to "individualize" the educational experience have led many educators to the study of psychological type differences. Many of these educators are attracted by the underlying assumption that the differences found between types represent alternative, but equally valid paths to the achievement of excellence. Jung (1921) postulated individual differences in a person's preferences for mode of perception. Within the realm of perception, he proposed that some are sensors , pre- ferring to glean knowledge as to what goes on around them from the physical senses, i.e., sight, hearing, touch, smell, and taste. These sensors strongly trust what their 10 LI experiences have told them. Jung stated that others are intuitives , preferring to perceive the world in terms of relationships, meanings and abstractions. These intuitors are often more concerned with what something means or might be than with what it is. Jung proposed that individuals also differ in the type of j udgment they rely upon. Some are thinkers , preferring to draw on their powers of logic and objectivity in making most decisions. Thinking types like best to utilize an impersonal approach in decision-making. The opposite type of person, Jung called feeling types, preferring to weigh all evidence within their own subjective value system as a basis for making decisions. Feeling types like best and trust most decisions where they have employed their own sub- jective criteria as to what is of highest value to them- selves and to others in the situation. The third individual difference in psychological type is in the basic orientation people take towards their environment. Extraverts are oriented toward the environ- ment, incorporating it into their own self -system and drawing energy from it. Introverts are oriented away from the surrounding environment. The environment is more alien to them. It takes energy away from them. In contrast to the extravert, they draw their energy from their own inner world of thought and ideas. Jung's analysis of these differ- ences in perception (Sensing and Intuition) and judgment (Thinking and Feeling) as they interact with basic 12 orientation towards the environment (Extraversion and Introversion) have been seen empirically to organize, within a powerful theoretical framework, many valuable observations of the educational process. In psychological type theory each of the four functions (Sensing, Intuition, Thinking, and Feeling) and orientations (Extraversion and Introversion, sometimes called attitudes) described by Jung is essential, and all of them are used by every individual. The functions and attitudes are not equally preferred, however, by all individuals. These basic differences in preferences lead to differing priorities of development, with the most preferred functions being differ- entiated earlier than the less preferred ones. Jung's psychological theory places great emphasis on consciousness. As precisely stated by Fordham, In attempting to divide human beings into recognizable types, Jung is dealing mainly with the psychology of consciousness; when a person is described as either extraverted or intro- verted, it means that his habitual conscious attitude is either the one or the other. A balanced attitude would include equally both extraversion and introversion, but it frequently happens that one attitude is developed and the other remains unconscious. No one, however, lives completely as one or the other, but mani- fests the unconscious attitude at times, though in an inferior way. (1966, p. 31) Thus, Jung's theoretical position was that the process of becoming a mature person, termed individuation, is one of life- long development taking different forms that are related to the person's preferences in the use of perception and judgment. Jung considered each psychological type to be 13 a "normal" process of maturation. Quenk (1978) very clearly emphasized this position, Thus Jung was quite explicit in stating that the attitude and function types are not "pure" but orienting structures which reflect the habitual way in which a person behaves. In energic, libido terms, the attitude and function type are the vehicles for the flow and amplitude of psychic energy. As a psychology of character, then, the Typology is not a static, classifi- catory scheme, but a dynamic process through which adaptation to the world occurs. (p. 5) Central to Jung's concept of maturation is "the process of becoming whole" (Fordham, 1966, p. 140). The key to this individuation process is the concept of making conscious the unconscious. Here Jung meant raising a person's level of awareness as to the existence of the differing approaches, which potentially increases the person's behavior repertoire and self -understanding. Most people use or rely heavily on one function; some use two. A highly differentiated or developed person uses three, and in rare cases, people exist who use all four functions (Sensing, Intuition, Thinking, and Feeling) . The use of all four functions is the end- point or goal of the individuation process. This bears similarity to Maslow's construct called self-actualization. According to Fordham' s interpretation (1966), the utiliza- tion of all four functions involves both "the individuation process, and the reconciliation of the opposing trends of one's nature" (p. 46). Thus, as the separate components of psychological type are explained in following sections it is important to bear in mind the dynamic, developmental under- pinnings of Jung's theory. 14 Extraversion, Introversion, and the Four Psychological Functions Jung considered the psychological type preferences to be dichotomous in nature--as "either-or" preferences. One either prefers the Introverted (I) or Extraverted (E) atti- tude, either Sensing (S) or Intuitive (N) perception, and either Thinking (T) or Feeling (F) judgments. There is a very logical ordering of the preferences from E-I to S-N to T-F. We direct our energy (E or I) to perceive a situa- tion (S or N) , and then to decide about it (T or F) . Jung called Sensing and Intuition the "irrational" functions because they are processes or modes of perception. They are not decision-making functions. They give us first the percepts or information that later we may or may not use in making decisions or judgments'. Perception is the process of cognition. We have utilized it when we say "I know" or "I see." Conversely, Jung called Thinking and Feeling the "rational" functions because they form the foundation of the decision-making process . They are two different avenues or approaches to coming to conclusions or judging about some- thing. It is necessary to acknowledge again that while Jung postulated the preference for E or I , S or N, and T or F to be "either-or" in nature, he did not mean that we utilize one preference to the total exclusion of the other. Fordham (1966) re-emphasizes this point, Since human nature is by no means simple, one rarely finds the absolutely pure type; often the main function is sufficiently clear to dub the 15 person a thinker, an intuitive and so on, but it is seconded by another function which modifies and blurs the picture. Jung in fact refers to his description of types as "somewhat Galtonesque family portraits," for human nature refuses to be classified in a precise and simple way. All the same, the concept of types has great practi- cal value as an aid to understanding in personal relationships and in education. (p. 45) In more specific terms, extraversion or introversion indicates the direction of the individual's flow of energy. A person who prefers the extraverted orientation is most "at home" when energy and attention are focused on the outer world of people and objects. Persons preferring intro- version are more comfortable in the inner world of their own thoughts and ideas. Kagan's "impulsive" and "reflec- tive" dimensions reflect the Extravert- Introvert difference in orientation. Jung (1921) states, The introvert's attitude is an abstracting one; at bottom, he is always intent on withdrawing libido from the object, as though he had to prevent the object from gaining power over him. The extravert, on the contrary, has a positive relation to the object. He affirms its importance to such an extent that his subjective attitude is constantly related to and oriented by the object. (p. 329) Next comes the perceptive function which is divided into the Sensing preference and the Intuitive preference (Sensing and Intuition are two of the four functions Jung refers to) . The person who prefers sensing perception likes best to take in information (i.e., perceive) through direct use of and reliance upon the physical senses. This type of perception is that which is most trusted by the individual. Alternately, the person who prefers intuitive perception 16 likes best to "see" or perceive the possibilities extant in a situation rather than the concrete realities of it. For example, Intuitive types are often described as people with great powers of imagination. Likewise, Sensing types are often described as being superbly practical. This is no surprise, as the intuitive person relishes in seeing "what might be" and the sensing person delights in clear percep- tion of "what is ." Finally, having perceived a situation, we come to con- clusions or decide about it. For this Jung said we utilize the judging function. The judging function is characterized by the preferences for Feeling judgments or for Thinking judgments (Feeling and Thinking are the other two of the four functions referred to by Jung) . While every individual has the potential for both kinds of judgment, each person tends to prefer or favor one kind over the other. This leads to greater and greater development of the favored way. Persons preferring Feeling judgment like best to rely on their own value system to guide their behavior. The con- scious and subjective weighing of things important to them and to others involved in a situation is a cornerstone of Feeling judgment. This process sheds light on the solution which can be most trusted and satisfying. Alternately, persons who prefer Thinking judgment trust and like best to decide based on what they believe to be the logic of the situation. Much emphasis is placed on the logical consequences of one decision as opposed to another (cause 17 and effect). The attempt at impersonal objectivity is a cornerstone of Thinking judgment. William James was probably noting type differences in the Thinking and Feeling functions when he postulated his tough-minded and tender- minded types . The Origin of Psychological Type Preferences The origin of the attitude and function types is not certain. It remains the object of scientific inquiry. Jung (1921) stated that at first we might be erroneously inclined to consider such differences "as mere idiosyn- crasies of character peculiar to individuals"; however, the differences in behavior are consistent enough to be highly predictable. He proceeded to state that these consistent differences are found across all ranks of society-- Sex makes no difference either; one finds the same contrast among women of all classes. Such a widespread distribution could hardly have come about if it were merely a question of conscious and deliberate choice of attitude. In that case, one would surely find one particular attitude in one particular class of people linked together by a common education and background and localized accordingly. But that is not so at all; on the contrary, the types seem to be distributed quite at random. (pp. 330-333) The type preferences show themselves in behavior very early. Von Franz and Hillman (1971) say in the crib, and Jun^ says on the first day of life. Jung seems to have ruled out Jung's highly detailed descriptions of the attitudes (E or I) and the four functions appear throughout Psychological Types (1921) , and in more condensed form in Chapter Eight of The Portable Jung, J. Campbell, 1971 18 any strict Mendelian inheritance pattern from parents as the origin of type preferences. In other words, he could observe no pattern wherein two extraverted parents produce an extraverted child, or two sensing parents a sensing child. Indeed, present-day data reveal no inheritance pat- terns either. Jung hints, however, that biological and physiological factors probably play a primary role-- In the same family one child is introverted, the other extraverted. Since the facts show that the attitude- type is a general phenomenon having an apparently random distribution, it cannot be a matter of conscious judgment or conscious intention, but must be due to some unconscious, instinctive cause. As a general psychological phenomenon, therefore, the type antithesis must have some kind of biological foundation . . . there are obviously individuals who have a greater capacity, or to whom it is more con- genial, to adapt in one way and not in another. It may well be that physiological causes of which we have no knowledge play a part in this. I do not think it improbable, in view of one's experience that a reversal of type often proves exceedingly harmful to the physiological well- being of the organism, usually causing acute exhaustion. (pp. 330-333) One avenue of inquiry which may eventually shed light on the origin of type preferences lies in investigation of the functioning of the right and left hemispheres of the brain. There may, for example, be a relationship between hemispheric dominance and preferred mode of perception (Sensing or Intuition) . It would seem possible that preference for sensing could be correlated with left- hemispheric dominance and preference for intuition with right-hemispheric dominance. Whatever the explanation, Jung clearly suspected a biological basis for the type 19 preferences; an inborn predisposition to develop in one direction rather than another. Both biology and environment play a role in the develop- ment of most human characteristics. Jung (1921) discussed how environmental influences (especially parents) can affect drastically what would be the normal type development of the child. But, he says these are extreme and abnormal cases and that As a rule, whenever such a falsification of type takes place as a result of parental influence, the individual becomes neurotic later, and can be cured only by developing the attitude consonant with his nature. (p. 332) Jung thus thought environment very important , with possibilities existing both for falsification of type and for helping develop a basic predisposition. In the Inner World of Childhood, Wickes (1966) provides a further example of the forces affecting the personality type of a very young child, The closer the bond between the parent and child the more the child is molded not only by the conscious but also the unconscious demands of the parent, reacting in many ways as he is expected to react instead of in accordance with his individual type. Such a confusion produces a sense of unreality and makes the integration of the personality a difficult task. (p. 130) Several writers have commented upon the effects the child's personality type has in the classroom, specifically in the relationship with the teacher. Wickes discussed the many ways in which the child is affected by school and ways that teachers or parents can ascertain (without 20 pigeonholing) clues as to psychological type of the child, and how to utilize that information to the best benefit of the child. Wickes (1966) offered the following anecdotal illustration of why clues as to the psychological type preferences of the child are important "tools" in the edu- cational process, I have known schools where it was possible to predict the success or failure of a child of pronounced type as he advanced from grade to grade and teacher to teacher. In one class he would be spoken of as a boy with an interesting mind and showing promise of great ability; in the next as impractical, dreamy, inaccurate. Or the reverse type of boy would be first depre- ciated as too ready, lacking in imagination and originality, and in the next grade praised as a fine, accurate, responsive, careful pupil. These verdicts were primarily based upon the type atti- tude of the teacher and upon the form of work which she valued. The same is true of parents. They feel rapport with the child whom they can understand. (pp. 134-135) Fordham (1966) states that it is of help to teachers to "realize that an introverted child, for instance, is not unhappy or unadapted if it does not join in activities with the same zest as extraverted pupils" (p. 45). She gives the following comparison of an extraverted and an intro- verted child: This [the extraverted child] is the type of child who is popular both with parents and teachers. He is spoken of as "well adjusted", and is often considered "brighter" than he really is because of his earlier development and his capacity to make a good impression. The introverted child is shy and hesitant. He dislikes all new situations, and even approaches new objects with caution, and some- times with fear. He prefers to play alone, and have one, rather than many friends. Because of the widespread preference for extraversion, such 21 introverted children often cause anxiety to parents, but they are just as "normal" and intel- ligent as the other type of child. They are thoughtful and reflective, and often have a rich imaginative life. What they need most is time to develop their less obvious gifts, and to learn to feel at home in the world. (p. 32) These external forces (parental and societal, as well as all life experiences) can be positive or negative in their influence on type development. In the lifetime of an individual, the formal education process has the potential for extensive positive influence. With respect to the institution of education, Inlow (1970) states that the goals of formal education and of humanism go hand in hand, "First and foremost, individual man is of supreme worth. Further- more, he is an emerging essence, with total personality ful- fillment constituting both his birthright and his commitment" (p. 29). Inlow further comments that learners themselves are an important source of any curriculum, "No curriculum, irrespective of its antecedents, is functional unless it relates meaningfully to the differing interests, abilities, and needs of learners" (pp. 117-118). Myers (1962) has long favored the smorgaasbord approach, where students of all types find their psychological differences both respected and nurtured, and where there is an atmosphere that fosters fuller personality development. Such an approach serves a dual purpose; every child is provided opportunities to develop all functions, as well as develop his own preferred functions to a higher level of expertise . 22 Lawrence (1979), in his practical guide to learning styles, provides examples using psychological type concepts in developing school curricula, and specifically for planning instructional strategies for the classroom which "honor the rights of every type" (p. 27). He clarifies how type con- cepts suggest solutions for two of education's most persis- tent and perplexing problems. School personnel across the country report lack of student motivation a major problem. Lawrence (1979) suggests breaking motivation down into four parts corresponding to the four dimensions of psychological type as follows: 1. The extraversion- introversion preference shows the broad areas of a student's natural interest 2. The sensing-intuition preference reveals basic learning style differences 3. The thinking- feeling dimension shows patterns of commitments and values of a student 4. The judging-perceiving dimension shows work habits. (p. 24) When these four natural motivators are taken into account, Lawrence suggests that teachers "can better direct student energies toward learning" (p. 24). Lawrence describes another problem which is increasingly becoming known to educators and educational researchers dealing with psychological type data. He refers to it as "perhaps the most crucial unrecognized problem of American education" (p. 27). Lawrence is pointing to the biases of instruction that may actually operate to the detriment of students who prefer extraverted sensing (ES types) . It is estimated (McCaulley, 1978; Myers, 1962) that these 23 extraverted sensing types make up about 70% of the general population, and hence approximately 707o of the average public school population. Two examples of these biases may serve to illustrate the problem. The classroom practice of presenting abstractions first and applications second appeals to and fits best the intuitive' s learning style, not that of the sensing type. Secondly, biases favoring both introversion and intuition may exist in reading instruction. Lawrence explains, Probably the young child first encounters it in the teaching of reading. Of course, reading is primarily an introverted activity; it is done quietly by oneself. And it is fundamentally an intuitive activity, involving abstractions — the printed symbols. But reading need not be taught as if all students were introverts and intuitives. The skills of reading can be and are being mastered by students of all types, and they all can leave school with a positive attitude toward reading. Tragically many students are alienated by their first encounters with reading instruc- tion. Type theory points to the probability that most of the alienated children are extraverted sensing types. There are data which indicate that the dropout rate is much higher among sensing children. (pp. 26-27) In addition, Lawrence and others have noted that standardized test constructors seem to be biased toward intuitive intelligence (manipulating symbols, abstract thinking, the drawing of inferences, describing "how to" rather than doing). As a group, IN types score highest on intelligence tests, followed by EN , IS , and finally ES types. Even textbooks seem to favor the intuitive way of viewing the world. According to Lawrence, "The record of American education in the twentieth century is a record of 24 neglect of sensing intelligence . . . the kind of intelligence possessed by the majority of American students" (1979, p. 27) . The problems raised by Lawrence are matters of concern for educators who wish to take an active role in fostering the total development of their students. Empirical research dealing directly with these issues is needed. Fuller under- standing of psychological type concepts and the process of type development offers great potential for developing better instructional strategies for individual learners. Both internal and external forces have a role in the normal "unfolding" of the developmental process. It is the favored mode for perceiving and for judging which is naturally the one most utilized by the child. Habitual use, through reinforcement, makes for further development and expertise in the use of the preferred way of perceiving and the preferred way of judging. This habitual use of one's preferences produces sets of characteristics, values and behaviors which a person of a given type shares in common with other people of the same psychological type. Humans, in general, seem to feel rapport with others who resemble them in the preferences for the psychological attitudes and functions. Van der Hoop's quote of the earth spirit's answer to Faust is illustrative, "That spirit thou resemblest, whom thou dost comprehend" (1939, p. 319). Von Franz and Hillman (1971) , in their treatise on psychological type, discussed the natural initial one- sidedness of the process 25 of development. In describing how our psychological type preferences affect our interpretation of the world around us Van der Hoop (1939) pointed out that our type differences have some far-reaching consequences- - One's immediate experience of one's fellowman proves itself at once to be much more influenced by one's own personality than is one's experience of a natural object, and the way in which this experience is dealt with is likewise still more under this influence, since the different modes of psychological objectif ication are sometimes determined by typical attitudes .... In the various schools of psychology, this influence can frequently be clearly demonstrated. . . . Men of different types are not all equally interested in scientific problems. . . . Although I do not maintain that this recognition of a one-sidedness in conscious orientation according to type offers an explanation of all the varieties in outlook, this differentiation does, nevertheless, make it possible to get a better understanding of the one- sidedness in these points of view. (pp. 319-320) An Interpretation of the Dominant and the Auxiliary Function Having dealt briefly with what the basic type preferences are, Jung's description of the dynamic inter- play of the preferences within a single individual can be reviewed. It is a gross oversimplification and probably an inaccuracy to deal with the effect of the preferences separately, without reference to one another, because the preferences have their joint effect in unison. To clarify this "orchestration" effect we must understand what Jung called the Dominant and the Auxiliary function. The Dominant function for any individual is theorized to be the most conscious and most developed function of the 26 four functions. Therefore, the Dominant function is either Sensing perception or Intuitive perception, or either Think- ing judgment or Feeling judgment. Also from the four functions, Jung postulated the existence of an auxiliary function. He stated (1921) that the auxiliary is "comple- mentary," "relatively unconscious," and "in every respect different from the nature of the primary [Dominant] func- tion" (pp. 405-406). We recall that the extraverted or introverted orientation indicates the direction of the individual's flow of energy; introversion toward the inner world of thought and ideas, extraversion toward the outer world of people, objects and events. Based on Jung's state- ment about the auxiliary function we can assume two things: (1) If the Dominant function is a judging function (thinking or feeling, whichever the person prefers), then the auxiliary function must be a perceptive function (sensing or intuition, whichever the person prefers) ; and if a perceptive function is dominant then the auxiliary function for that person must lie in a judging function; and (2) If the Dominant function for a person is extraverted, then the auxiliary function must be introverted; and if the Dominant function is introverted, then the auxiliary function must be extraverted. With regard to this Quenk (1978) stated, "A function cannot be both extraverted and introverted. Its directionality must be unitary" (p. 8). Myers (1962) had earlier come to this same deduction when she and her mother, Katharine C. Briggs, in the development 27 of the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI) added the J - P Index to indicate whether the person uses a perceptive function (S or N) or a judging function (T or F) in the extraverted world (that shown most to others). As shown in Figure 2, the judging or perceptive attitude identifies (points to) the Dominant function for Extraverts and the auxiliary function for Introverts. According to Myers (1962) , persons who prefer the extraverted orientation (prefer to direct energy to the outer world of people, places and objects) extravert their Dominant function. Introverts (prefer to direct energy to their own world of thoughts and ideas) introvert their Dominant function. The extraverts introvert their auxiliary function, and the introverts extravert their auxiliary function. According to Quenk (1978), It also follows that an extravert is more confident of his dominant function, especially as he receives confirmation from others. An introvert, because he extraverts his auxiliary [or second best] function, receives confirma- tion from the environment only for his auxiliary; he must rely on his self-awareness to ascertain his dominant function. . . . Jung has stated that extraverts fear their inner world just as introverts fear the outer world. (p. 10) In line with this argument Plaut (1972) found that intro- verts have less confidence than extraverts in determining their dominant function. In direct relation to this, Myers (1962) pointed out that all of us must live in both "inner" and "outer" worlds. Some prefer the outer world; some prefer the inner world. 28 According to Myers (1962) , the excursion into the least liked place is delegated to the person's auxiliary function. With regard to the auxiliary function she stated, If he [the extravert] has no useful development of an auxiliary process, he will have little or no inner life, which will make him an extreme extravert, and better-balanced associates will find him superficial. ... If the introvert has no useful development of an auxiliary process, his outer life will be a very awkward, accidental and uncomfortable affair. (pp. 60-61) On the issue of extraversion, introversion, and the dominant and the auxiliary function Quenk (1978) further stated, The well-developed function is that function which has a definite directionality to its energic charge. Directionality is an all-or-none phenome- non. Metaphorically, one can only go in one direction at a time. Thus, as the functions become specialized their directionality can be seen as central to their development. (p. 12) The "evolution" process of psychological type has been described by Van der Hoop (1939) as follows: A first point of difference between people of the same type is found in the stage of development which they have reached. In every type there is a simple form, in which the differentiation of the prevailing function has only just begun, and its modes of adaptation are still being tentatively tried out, although a clear prefer- ence for typical forms of adaptation can already be observed. At a later stage the dominating function has found its forms, controlling these with great assurance. Anything which is not in accord is, at this stage, suppressed. With a few people there follows a still further stage, in which the other functions are permitted more development, to compensate for any one-sidedness , and the pronounced typical picture is again modi- fied to some extent by the unfolding of a fuller and richer expression of human nature. (p. 92) In a lecture in Zurich, Jung (1928) further explicated the interplay of the functions in the natural unfolding process 29 of type development. He gave several behavior-based examples for recognizing functions at differing levels of development , Whether a function is differentiated or not can be recognized from its strength, stability, consistency, reliability and adaptedness. But inferiority in a function is often not so easy to recognize or to describe. An essential criterion is its lack of self-sufficiency and consequent dependence on people and circumstances, its disposing us to moods and crotchetiness , its unreliable use, its suggestable and labile character. The inferior function always puts us at a disadvantage because we cannot direct it but rather are its victim. As a final point of clarification, Figures 1 and 2 may serve as aides to remembering the proposed nature of psy- chological type as a developmental schema. FLOW OF ENERGY PERCEPTION JUDGMENT Extraverted orientation (E) Introverted orientation (I) Sensing (S) Intuition (N) ■ .a Thinking1 !\ (t) Feeling (F) THE FOUR FUNCTIONS ATTITUDE TOWARD ENVIRONMENT Judging _v attitude (J) Perceptive .^ attitude (P) Figure 1. Diagram illustrating the basic psychological preferences . 30 For the Extravert For the Extravert: For the Introvert: For the Introvert ->l 1 cn |-j| cu rH M O LO CU r) O M 1 Pm| 1 M 1 H E cu U a r-H oo H o a O o CN 1 cu rH H rH m u n 0) rH <4H CU u a CO o vO i en CO i-i a\ 00 VO 1 CU (0 i— i 3 4-1 H| 1 H| 1 H| • H| u CJ ON CN CTi 1 o (fl o\ CT\ r>« 1 m 0) vO ,0 4-1 O cu d o o CN O 1 -* 1 M O >i rH <-t •-i i CU B m u S 21 1 0) SB| 1 55 1 1 1 d CU 21 u o H o 00 H 1 i-i CD O 1 e o CN I CU i-H CJ H (U r-H CD H 1 <4H sr a M O CU rH 0) a) d n h 4H cu a. cu CU u MH r4 a> 00 cn cu P. U-l d CO u ro | O 1 cu O 1 0 cu o. tn i a) 4J 00 1 u CU vO i u 4-1 rH 4J W| a) W| 1 CQ| 1 1 en ot| IH J2 )-i u O 00 CTi 1 cu CN 1 CO o> ] >, •H ON I T> o\ 1 01 P» 1 U o> H o rH cu in cn B CJ > cu cn cn en en u cu r*» i cu o l 01 O i a) o 00 O 1 oo CN 1 oo -d- 1 00 e d H 1 c r-i 1 c ^ 1 d a) cd n3 cfl u M H| 1 ! i-i H| 1 i u H| 1 u M| 0 cu -H 1 ai 00 I 0) rH | 0) rH M O 1 u O 1 1-1 CN 1 M ■ t-H CO CO O rH -3" CO IX M| 78 a significant relationship between D4JP and MJS score. The relationship appears non-linear. Group 3 had significantly- higher MJS scores than did group 2. For J-P then, the students expressing only moderate preference for J or P, with slight word-pair/phrase discrepancy, and moderate split-half difference, had the highest scores on the MJS. Hypothesis 2.1 received support. Students who reported the highest preferences for either Extraversion or Intro- version (on D1EI) had significantly higher overall class rank and overall grade point average than students expressing lower preferences for E or I . The same trend existed for academic class rank and academic grade point average, but results did not reach the .05 level of significance. Students who reported the highest preferences for either Sensing or Intuition (on D1SN) had significantly higher academic class rank and academic grade point average than students reporting lower preferences for Sensing or Intuition. The same trend existed for overall class rank and overall grade point average, but results were not significant at the .05 level. Students expressing the highest preferences for either Thinking or Feeling (on D1TF) had significantly higher overall and academic class rank and overall and academic grade point averages than students expressing only moderate or slight preferences for T or F. Another significant non- linear relationship appeared for the J-P scale as it did in H (see D1JP on Table 5). 79 ^ 00 rH m s-*. cn <-N CM O cn in O H H in w| a 1 • o • o cn o CM o p~ ^-^ CM • CM • CM • jj ^> ^ • o • »' H ^ m ^ iH v— ' • ' • v-x m ^~^ • "s"^ ^ w| 4J c cn CO 00 CO J0 \D cn cn CM cn ^^' cn cn cm en cn cn CO CO p» cn C > a fi CJ > c cj a > O > a > CJ cn o CO o CO CO CO O CO CO co 0 Cfl o cfl 0 cfl o> w u u H u j= -3 <# . ^3 <-3 c3 ■-3 ^ • ^ . ^3 ^) • 4-1 cn CO cn cfl cn CO cO Cfl cn cfl CO 0 H cn M • z cn Z ta cn fn cn Pn • to • P-. CO CH P-4 • a, W CO W D, w CO . rH H rH • rH H rH rH rH .-i rH iH • 1-t • rH rH rH rH . K o a O 00 Q a P 00 P o O O Q 00 Q OO Q u P 00 P 00 80 Students who reported only a moderate preference for either the judging or perceptive attitude had significantly higher overall class rank and overall grade point average than students reporting higher and lower preferences for J or P. Hypothesis 2.2 was only partially supported (see D2JP, Table 6). No significant relationships emerged for word-pair/phrase discrepancies with academic variables for the E-I, S-N, or T-F scales of the MBTI . A significant relationship existed for the J-P scale. In the post hoc comparisons, group 4 (students with the largest word-pair/ phrase discrepancy for J or P) came out highest on all four academic variables. In 3 of 4 post hoc comparisons, group 1 (students with no word-pair/phrase discrepancy on J or P) were second highest on academic variables. The relation- ship thus appears curvilinear with students highest and lowest in word-pair/phrase discrepancies on J-P faring best on academic variables . Hypothesis 2.3 was not supported. No significant relationships emerged between x-y half differences and academic variables . Hypothesis 2.4 dealt with total discrepancy scores (D4EI to D4JP) which combines strength of preference, word- pair/phrase discrepancies, and x-y half differences for E-I, S-N, T-F, and J-P, and combined discrepancy scores (Dl to D4) where Dl combines strength of reported preference for all four MBTI scales, D2 combines word-pair/phrase \D *-N VO >-% ^^ vo m cn in on m u>\a 1 • o • O as o rH • m *•» in ^ rH • u *-n I-l •-s O /-> cn /^ • t-i o> in • in w m en c cj a > CJ o a > CJ > f3 h a S-i 0) 0 CO cfl cfl o cfl cfl cfl 0 CO O cfl eu cfl CU JZ M u U u > M > 4-1 m3 <^J l£ . ca . •a <^J <& > • >-) CO n co |-3 a n Cm • CO CO w P- m a H cfl cfl a 33 CN N H CN rH CN • CN • CN vT H O m o CN O CM O w| 0- | o • O • CN • CN • CO . -N OT| 4-1 C CO CM CO CN CO CN CO CN CO co co - o cu 4-i \aj & a Q. P. a p. a a. •h cn cn u-i 00 oo oo oo 00 oo 00 00 ■4-4 -H CO 14-1 •H 2 S-i CU A A A A A A A A cu CU 1— 1 r-4 i— 1 tH H i-l rH i-H on -H c o c c •H CO o w 0 o a- a. D. a. O. a. 0. a cn a. o ^ c c oo oo 00 oo 00 oo oo oo -K CN CN CN 00 00 CO t-H CO -a- CO O o o .-H o ^H CM M O o o O o O CM CO O CN CN 1-1 O o O 0.1 O o o O o O tH ^. ^ ^ ^^ ^^ ^ H c^ M3 a> >-' co cn cn cn -3- cn m co c CJ C3 > o cn cn e cfl cfll cfl 0 cfl cfl cfl o cfl a) co S-l 1 u U -C 0) C "3 1 ^ . ^ ^a ^ • ca • 4-1 M -H col CO CO cn ro CO 0 0) E H Z Wl z • i t-> cn in 1 H i-H i o Ln CO O 1 CT\ O i cn o co| o-l rH w u 00 a-. 00 o> CN rH * rH CO h CN CN '-' cn cn ^r cn m cn cn cn cn cn CO CO CO CO 0 ^^ s v*' ^~^ **^ y~' w O o o o o vP vp CN CN CN 1 CN CN rH .H Z| CN CN CN CN CN .H rH en 01 1) •H M u en 11 4-J 0) 4-1 a) -X3 C J= c £ 3 2 CO 3 Z CO 4-1 C 1 a o Z P* Z 1 Z w -H Z M -H P. e/3 1 H CO 1 CO 0 E 1 co o e >> H 1 H -J- 1 vD rH VD CN 00 l-l /->> ^H H rH rH ^H .—i CN CN CO H <-i .H CN CN cn en U-. UH uj M-i CO 1-4 U-l UH uh U-l U-l u-i U-l U-l U-l UH UH a. o o 0 O ■U X o O 0 0 O O o 0 O O 0 3 o a O tn en tn CO 4-1 ■>«. en en en en en cn cn en en en en M H H H rH 1-1 .-i rH H .-1 H j-i T-l rH r-l rH rH oo CO CO CO CO u a. cd CO CO n) CO CO CO CO cfl CO Cfl 4-1 4J 4-1 4-1 C 1 4-1 4-1 4-1 ■U 4-) 4J 4-1 4J 4-1 4J 4-1 u O 0 o 0 ■H T3 0 O o o o 0 0 O 0 o O 3 4-1 4-1 4-J 4-1 O 3 4-1 4-1 4-1 4-1 4-1 4-1 4-1 4-1 4-1 4-1 4-1 o P. U-l 4-1 4-1 4-1 4-1 c 4-1 4-1 4-1 4-1 4-1 4-1 4-1 4-1 4-1 4-1 4-1 a c C a >, o C c c c c C C c c c c U •H iH •H •H T— 1 ■H •H •H •H 1-4 ■H i-l •H T-t •H •H 0 o o O O c • O 0 O 0 o O o O O O O U-l a. a a cu O U-l U-l P4 Cu O. ex a. a D. a. cu Cu Cu C en en Bl tn tn i-i 01 tn en en en tn cn cn cn en en 3 01 0) 01 ai aj -a QJ 0) 01 01 CU 0J cu cu cu cu a) o c c c a -a C a c (3 C c a C C C C -a ■H -H •H •H 3 a) •H ■H •H •H -H ■H •H •H i-l •H •H j*s -a x X X h a. X X XI X X X X X X X X cfl B B e E o >> e s B e e e s e e e e 01 o o o o a 4J O o 0 o o 0 o O 0 o o u u cj u O i-t CJ CJ o CJ CJ CJ CJ CJ CJ a CJ « c ^H CN CO -) >~> •-3 H CN ro Q Q a + + + U, to Pi. H H H X — i CN n QJ Q Q Q -a e + + + z^ M Z S3 2 cn Q ^ LO t/1 e/3 CJ rH CN cn + CI a Q Q cfl CN PL, ai u + + + Q H H M + u M W W en r^ CN cn r-l •H rH Q CN Q < m NJ TJ 1113 2650 11.94 ESTJ ESFJ ENFJ ENTJ 28.43 N-- 1395 "» = 14.97 N= 1302 °° = 13.97 N=336 0/o = 3.61 N = 366 °o=3.93 on C o 0 TP FP FJ 1782 2447 2441 19.12 26.26 26.19 z IN 1104 11.85 o EN IS ES 1866 2172 4178 20.02 23.30 44.83 NOTES: 116 Myers-Briggs Type Indicator Table C3. Comparison of 485 high school students with Myers1 Pennsylvania high school student sample (N = 9,320). SENSING TYPES INTUITIVE TYPES with THINKING with FEELING with FEELING with THINKING Type Table Legend: % = percent of total choosing this group who fall into this type. I = ratio of percent of type in group to 7o in sample. ISTJ ISFJ INFJ INTJ E 1 241 244 49.69 50.31 0.77* 1.43* N = 32 N-43 N=14 N=4 c °°=6.60 '• =8.87 °°=2.89 °° =0.82 O s 311 64.12 0.94 I =0.95 I =1.30 I =1.61 I =0.32^ z O N 174 35.88 1.13 z T 154 31.75 0.67* TO o < F J 331 182 68.25 37.53 1.30* ISTP ISFP INFP INTP 0.69* N = 37 N=58 N=45 N=ll P 303 62.47 1.38* °o =7.63 I =1.83* '. =11.96 I =2.22* °==9.28 I =2.38* °o=2.27 I =0.64 TO n IJ IP 93 151 19.18 31.13 1.06 1.83* < EP EJ ST 152 89 119 192 31.34 18.35 24.54 39.59 1.10 0.50* 0.75* 1.11 ESTP ESFP ENFP ENTP SF N = 20 '• "4. 12 N = 64 °o =13.20 N=57 %=11.75 N=ll °° =2.27 ■70 n NF NT 139 35 28.66 7.22 1.70* 0.48* I =0.63" I =1.41// I =1.55* I =0.46// ■o SJ 132 27.22 0.64* < SP 179 36.91 1.45* NP 124 25.57 1.28# X) > < m NJ TJ 50 75 10.31 15.46 0.86 ESTJ ESFJ ENFJ ENTJ 0.54* N= 30 "• = 6.19 I =0.41* N = 27 °° = 5.57 I =0.40* N = 23 °°=4.74 I =1.32 N=9 °o=1.86 I =0.47" on C o o TP FP FJ 79 224 107 16.29 46.19 22.06 0.85 1.76* 0.84" z O IN EN IS ES 74 100 170 141 15.26 20.62 35.05 29.07 1.29" 1.03 1.50* 0.65* NOTES: concerning symbols following the selection ratios: " implies significance at the .05 level, i.e., chi sq.> 3.8 // implies significance at the .01 level, i.e., chi sq.p> 6.6 * implies significance at the .001 level, i.e., chi sq.> 10.8 - (underscore) indicates Fisher's Exact Probability used instead of chi-square. APPENDIX D RESULTS AND DISCUSSION OF SELECTION RATIO TYPE TABLE (SRTT) ANALYSES Additional analyses of the four categories or groups of each discrepancy index were needed to properly interpret the analysis of variance results presented in Chapter IV. If significant differences in the basic type preferences of subjects existed between the four groups for each discrep- ancy index, the analysis of variance results would be diffi- cult to interpret. For example, if for D1SN, category 1 and category 4 significantly discriminated students who did best academi- cally from those who performed less well, then for D1SN, category 1 (students who reported a strong preference for Sensing or Intuition) and category 4 (students who reported only a slight preference for Sensing or Intuition) must be looked at closely to make sure category 1 is not predominantly Intuitive types and category 4 is not predominantly Sensing types. If this were true the picture would become very blurred. Two different interpretations would be supported. The finding might result from the fact that Intuitive types tend to out-perform Sensing types on academic variables, or it could be that students with a clear, consistent, and well-developed preference, either for Sensing or for Intuition, do better academically than those students who 117 118 have not developed a strong preference to use in perceiving. In reality, the finding might be explained in part by both factors and by other factors as well. The SRTT also helps answer the previously posed ques- tion as to whether grades get lower as Sensing is more strongly reported. Three sample SRTT's are provided in Tables Dl through D3, and their implications discussed. Table Dl is the selection ratio type table (SRTT) between group 1 and group 3 for D1EI (strength of preference for Extraversion or Introversion) , the students in group 1 reporting very strong preferences for E or I and group 3 reporting only moderate preference. This SRTT table is of interest because group 1 was significantly higher in overall class rank than group 3. Since in past studies Introverts, in general, have been found to make higher grades than Extraverts, it was necessary to make sure that group 1 did not contain significantly more Introverts than group 3. It did not. Hence, this lends further credence to the finding that it is strength of preference for either E or I which accounts for the difference, not preference for I over E, per se. Table D2 is the SRTT between groups 2 and 4 for D1TF (strength of preference for thinking or feeling) , with group 2 reporting clear preferences for T or F, group 4 very slight preference. This table is of interest because group 2 had significantly higher Moral Judgment Scale scores than group 4. In group 2 we find 28 thinking types compared with 119 106 feeling types. Group 4 had 68 thinking types and 69 feeling types. Thus in the low MJS group (group 4) prefer- ence for Thinking and for Feeling are equally distributed. However, in the high MJS group (group 2) we find a signifi- cantly higher proportion of Feeling types than Thinking types. It appears that the high number of ENFP ' s in group 2 accounts for this difference. There are, nevertheless, 28 students in the high MJS group with very strong preference for Thinking. This does not change the interpretation that high preference for either T or F goes with high level of moral reasoning, but the SRTT does raise the question as to why the feeling types (particularly ENFP) are so highly represented in the high MJS group. A look at the level 5 and 6 responses for the MJS vignettes may shed some light on this. They appear to be the type of response which would appeal to feeling judgment. They call for a subjective weighing of values important to persons involved in the situation, rather than reasoning based on clear-cut principles of right or wrong, or legal contrivance. While not within the scope of the present investigation, this could be determined in future research on type preference and the MJS. It is possible that the very task of completing a moral judgment questionnaire appeals more to feeling than thinking types. Table D3 shows the SRTT for groups 1 and 3 on D4SN (combining strength of preference, word-pair/phrase dis- crepancy, and x-y half difference for Sensing or Intuition). 120 As predicted, group 1 (high preference with low word-pair/phrase discrepancy and x-y difference) had sig- nificantly higher overall and academic grade point average than group 3 (moderate preference with high word-pair/phrase discrepancy and x-y difference). We do not find a signifi- cant difference in the proportion of Sensing to Intuitive types in these two groups. Thus, this lends further support to the interpretation that strong, clear and consistent preference for one's perceptive function (whether it be Sensing or Intuition) goes with academic achievement. If there had been a higher than usual proportion of Intuitives in group 1 this would have made the above interpretation less credible, because preference for Intuition has been found to be positively correlated with academic success. To some extent this offers evidence for the question posed by Myers (1962) and explored by Weychert (1975) . This was the question as to whether grades get lower as preference for Sensing increases. Since we find an equal proportion of types reporting high preference for Sensing as we do types reporting high preference for Intuition in the group with highest academic and overall grade point average, we find no significant trend for grades being lower when preference for Sensing is strong. 121 Myers-Briggs Type Indicator Table Dl. SRTT analysis for D1EI groups 1 and 3. SENSING TYPES INTUITIVE TYPES with THINKING with FEELING with FEELING with THINKING ISTJ N = 3 °»=6.25 I =0.72 ISTP N = 3 %=6.25 I =0.72 ESTP N-l '• -2.0 1 = 0. 67 ESTJ N-0 % -0.0 I =0.0" ISFJ %=6.25 I =0.67 N INFJ N=l •■.=2.08 I =1.12 ISFP N=5 °o =10.42 I =0.76 ESFP N=5 °°=10.42 I =0.99 INFP N=4 °o=8.33 I =0.84 ESFJ N=l "'.=2.08 I =0.30 ENFP N=ll °o =22.92 I =2.46" ENFJ N=8 D/o=16.67 I =5.37# INTJ N=0 °„ =0.0 I =0.0 INTP N=l °o=2.08 I =1.68 ENTP N=l °°=2.08 I =0.84 ENTJ N=l %=2.08 I =1.68 Type Table Legend: % = percent of total choosing this group who fall into this type. I = self selection index: ratio of per- cent of type in group to 7o in sample. N % I 28 20 21 27 10 38 17 31 7 13 18 10 58.33 41.67 43.75 56.25 20.83 79.17 35.42 64.58 14.58 27.08 37.50 20.83 1.27 0.77 0.62* 1.89* 0.59 1.23 0.86 1.09 0.71 0.81 1.47 1.02 ST 7 14.58 0.49" SF 14 29.17 0.72 NF 24 50.00 2.06* NT 3 6.25 1.12 SJ 7 14.58 0.43// SP 14 29.17 0.81 m X NP NJ 17 10 35.42 20.83 1.54 3.05# > TJ 4 8.33 0.42 en TP 6 12.50 0.81 FP 25 52.08 1.20 FJ 13 27.08 1.28 IN 6 12.50 0.91 EN 21 43.75 2.71* IS 14 29.17 0.72 bb 7 14.58 0.49" NOTES: concerning symbols following the selection ratios: " implies significance at the .05 level, i.e., chi sq. > 3.8 // implies significance at the .01 level, i.e., chi sq.>6.6 * implies significance at the .001 level, i.e., chi sq.>10.8 - (underscore) indicates Fisher's Exact Probability used instead of chi-square. 122 AAyers-Briggs Type Indicator Table D2 . SRTT analysis for D1TF groups 2 and 4. SENSING TYPES INTUITIVE TYPES withTHINKING with FEELING with FEELING with THINKING Type Table Legend: °L = percent of total choosing this group who fall into this type. I = self selection index: ratio of per- cent of type in group to 7o in sample. N % I ISTJ ISFJ INFJ INTJ E 1 69 65 51.49 48.51 1.22 0.84 N = 8 N = o, N=5 N=0 c °°=5.97 "° "6.72 °°=3.73 °° =0.0 o o S 77 57.46 0.80" I =0.48 I =0.92 I =1.28 I =0.0 z O N 57 42.54 1.53" 2 T 28 20.90 0.42* 73 o < m F J 106 43 79.10 32.09 1.57* ISTP ISFP INFP INTP 0.76 N=7 N = 18 N = 16 N=2 on P 91 67.91 1.18 °<- = 5.22 "• =13.43 % =11.94 °.=1.49 TO IJ 22 16.42 0.68 I =0.51 I =1.08 I =1.82 I =0.34 m -a IP 43 32.09 0.96 < EP 48 35.82 1.49" m EJ ST 21 24 53 15.67 17.91 39.55 0.86 0.48* 1.13 ESTP ESFP ENFP ENTP SF N=5 '.= 3.73 N = 19 '■• = 14.18 N=24 % = 17.91 N=0 \ =0.0 -o n NF NT 53 4 39.55 2.99 2.58* 0.24# I =0.57 I =1.39 I = 4.91* I =0.0 ■o SJ 28 20.90 0.64" < SP 49 36.57 0.93 x NP 42 31.34 1.72" 3D > < m NJ TJ 15 14 11.19 10.45 1.18 ESTJ ESFJ ENFJ ENTJ 0.42# N-- 4 %= 2.99 I = 0.37 N = 7 °o = 5.22 I = 1.02 N= 8 % = 5.97 I =2.73 N=2 °o = 1.49 I = 0.51 C O o TP FP FJ 14 77 29 10.45 57.46 21.64 0.42# 1.75* 1.24 z o IN EN IS ES 23 34 42 35 17.16 25.37 31.34 26.12 1.12 2.04# 0.74 0.87 NOTES: concerning symbols following the selection ratios: " implies significance at the .05 level, i.e., chi sq.>3.8 // implies significance at the .01 level, i.e., chi sq. >6.6 * implies significance at the .001 level, i.e., chi sq.> 10.8 - (underscore) indicates Fisher's Exact Probability used instead of chi-square. 123 Myers-Briggs Type Indicator Table D3 . SRTT analysis for D4SN groups 1 and 3. SENSING TYPES INTUITIVE TYPES with THINKING with FEELING with FEELING withTHINKING ISTJ ISFJ INFJ INTJ N=10 N=6 N=l N=l °o=14.29 ••. -8.57 %=1.43 % =1.43 I =3.16# I =1.17 I =0.28 I =1.26 iSTP ISFP INFP INTP N-5 N=9 N=5 N= 2 °o = 7.14 °o =12.86 % =7 . 14 °0 = 2.86 I =1.05 I =1.03 I =0.79 I = 5.06 ESTP ESFP ENFP ENTP N-2 N = 3 N = 6 N= 2 °° = 2.86 °o = 4.29 °o=8.57 °„= 2.86 I =0.51 I =0.23# I =1.01 I = 1.26 £577 ESFJ ENFJ ENTJ N= 10 N=5 N = 2 N = 1 %~ 14.29 °= = 7.14 % = 2.86 %= 1.43 I =2.30" I = 1.05 I =0.72 I = 1.26 Type Table Legend: % = percent of total choosing this group who fall into this type. I = self selection Index: ratio of per- cent of type in group to 70 in sample. N % I E 31 44.29 0.83 1 39 55.71 1.19 S 50 71.43 1.04 N 20 28.57 0.90 T 33 47.14 1.67# F 37 52.86 0.74# J 36 51.43 1.42" P 34 48.57 0.76" IJ 18 25.71 1.42 IP 21 30.00 1.04 EP 13 18.57 0.53" EJ 18 25.71 1.42 ST SF NF NT SJ SP NP NJ TJ TP FP FJ 27 23 14 6 31 19 15 5 22 11 23 14 38 57 1.67" 32 86 0.73 20 00 0.75 8 57 1.69 44 29 1.78// 27 14 0.62" 21 43 1.05 7 14 0.63 31 43 2.42* 15 71 1.03 32 86 0.68" 20 00 0.86 IN 9 12.86 0.81 EN 11 15.71 0.99 IS 30 42.86 1.38 ES 20 28.57 0.77 NOTES: concerning symbols following the selection ratios: " implies significance at the .05 level, i.e., chi sq. > 3.8 // implies significance at the .01 level, i.e., chi sq. > 6.6 * implies significance at the .001 level, i.e., chi sq.> 10.8 - (underscore) indicates Fisher's Exact Probability used instead of chi-square. BIBLIOGRAPHY Allen, J. S., & Kainz , R. I. Selection ratio type table program. Gainesville, Fla. : Center for Applications of Psychological Type, Inc., 1976. Anast, P. Personality determinants of mass media prefer- ences. Journalism Quarterly, 1966, 4_3, 729-732. Beare, D. Self -concept and the adolescent L/LD student. TPGA Journal, 1975, 4, 22-29. Black, W. F. Self-concept as related to achievement and age in learning disabled children. Child Development, 1974, 45, 1137-1140. Bruner, J., & Starkey, J. Interpersonal relationships and the self -concept . DeKalb, 111 . : Northern Illinois University, 1974 . (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 089 515) Busby, W. A., Fillmer, H. T., & Smittle, P. Interrelation- ship between self -concept , visual perception and reading disabilities. Journal of Experimental Educa- tion, 1974, 42, 1-6. Campbell, J. (Ed.) The portable Jung (R.F.C. Hull, Trans.). New York: Penguin Books, 1971. Carlyn, M. An assessment of the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. Journal of Personality Assessment, 1977, 41_(5) , 461-473. Center for Applications of Psychological Type, Inc. Bibliography: Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. Gainesville Fla. , 1981. Coan, R. W. Review of the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. In 0. K. Buros (Ed.), The Eighth Mental Measurements Yearbook. Highland Park, N.J ~ The Gryphon Press, 1978 Fordham, F. An introduction to Jung's psychology (3rd ed.). Baltimore^ Md. : Penguin Books, 1966. Fox, D. J. The research process in education. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston, 1969. 124 125 Giffin, M. How does he feel? In E. Schloss (ed.), The educator's enigma: The adolescent with learning dis- abilities^ San Rafael, Calif. : Academic Therapy Publications, 1971. Gilligan, C, Kohlberg, L. , Lerner, J., & Belenky, M. Moral reasoning about sexual dilemmas : The development of an interview and scoring system. In Technical report of the Commission on Obscenity and Pornography (Vol. 1) . Washington, D.C. : U.S. Government Printing Office, 1971. Golanty-Koel, R. The relationship of psychological types and mass media preferences to the values of non-academic high school students (Doctoral dissertation, University of California, 1977) . Dissertation Abstracts Inter- national, 1978, 38, 4683~rAT (University Microfilms No. 73-29, 395) Gordon, S. Reversing a negative self-image. In L. Anderson (Ed.), Helping the adolescent with the hidden handicap. Los Angeles: Academic Therapy Publications, 1970. Gough, H. G. The Adjective Check List as a personality assessment research technique. Psychological Reports, 1960, 6, 107-122. Gough, H. G. , & Heilbrun, A. B., Jr. Manual for the Adjective Check List. Palo Alto, Calif.: Consulting Psychologists Press, 1965. Gow, D. W. Dyslexic adolescent boys: Classroom remediation is not enough. Bulletin of the Orton Society, 1974, 24, 154-163. Guttinger, H. I. Patterns of perceiving: Factors which affect individualized reading instruction at the high school level. Paper presented at the meeting of the American Educational Research Association, Chicago, April 1974. Guttinger, H. I., & Hines , V. A. Field testing and diffusion of an experiment in developmental, individ- ualized reading at the middle and high school level's" (Research Monograph No. 20) . Gainesville, Fla. : P. K. Yonge Laboratory School, University of Florida, 1977. Hicks, L. E. Some properties of ipsative, normative and forced-choice measures. Psychological Bulletin, 1970, 74, 167-184. 126 Hogan, R. T. Moral development: An assessment approach. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of California, Berkeley, 1967. Hogan, R. T. A dimension of moral judgment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1970, 35(2), 205-212. — Inlow, G. M. Education: Mirror and agent of change. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Inc. , 1970. Jung, C. G. [Psychological types] (G. G. Baynes, trans. & R. F. C.'Hull, Ed7) . Princeton, N.J. : Princeton University Press, 1971. (Originally published, 1921.) Jung, C. G. A psychological theory and types . Lecture delivered at Congress of Swiss Psychiatrists, Zurich, 1928. Kainz, R. I. An investigation of response style in the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator, a Jungian typology inventory. Unpublished masters thesis, University of Florida, 1978. Ker linger, F. N. Foundations of behavioral research (2nd ed.). New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1973. Kohlberg, L. The development of modes of moral thinking and choice in the years ten to sixteen. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Chicago, 1958. Kohlberg, L. Moral development and identification. In H. W. Stevenson (Ed.), Child psychology. (62nd Year- book of the Study of Education) (Pt. I). Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1963. Kohlberg, L. Development of moral character and moral ideology. In M. L. Hoffman & L. W. Hoffman (Eds.), Review of child development research (Vol. 1). New York: Russell Sage Foundation, 1964. Kohlberg, L. Stage and sequence: The cognitive-developmental approach to socialization. In D. A. Goslin (Ed.), Handbook of socialization theory and research. Chicago: Rand McNally, 1969. Kohlberg, L. From is to ought: How to commit the naturalistic fallacy and get away with it in the study of moral development. In T. Mischel (Ed.), Cognitive development and epistemology . New York: Academic Press, 1971. 127 Kohout, F. J. Statistics for social scientists: A coordinated learning system! New York: John Wiley & Sons, 1974. Labovitz, S. The assignment of numbers to rank order categories, American Sociological Review, 1970, 35, 515-524. ~ Lawrence, G. D. People types and tiger stripes: A practi- cal guide to learning styles. Gainesville, Fla. : Center for Applications of Psychological Type, Inc., 1979. Leviton, H. , & Kiraly, J. Achievement and self-concept in young L.D. children. Academic Therapy, 1975, 10, 453-455. — Maitland, K. A., & Goldman, J. R. Moral judgment as a function of peer group interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1974, 30(5), 699- Marascuilo, L. A. Statistical methods for behavioral science research. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1971. McCaulley, M. H. The Myers-Briggs Type Indicator and the teaching- learning process. Paper presented at the meeting of the American Educational Research Associ- ation, Chicago, April 1974. (ERIC Reproduction Service No. ED 093 965) McCaulley, M. H. Personality variables: Modal profiles that characterize various fields of science. In M. B. Rowe (chair) , Birth of new ways to raise a scientifi- cally literate society: Research that may help. Symposium presented at the meeting of the American Association for the Advancement of Science, Boston, February 1976. (a) McCaulley, M. H. Psychological types in engineering: Implications for teaching. Engineering Education, 1976, 66(7), 729-736. (b) McCaulley, M. H. The Myers longitudinal medical study (HRA contract 231-76-0051 Monograph 2). Gainesville, Fla. : Center for Applications of Psychological Type, 1977. 128 McCaulley, M. H. Application of the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator to medicine and other health professions (Monograph 1, Contract no. 231-76-0051, Health Resources Administration, DHEW) . Gainesville, Fla.: Center for Applications of Psychological Type, 1978. McDonnell, M. The comparative effects of teacher reinforce- ment of self-esteem and academic achievement on affective variables and achievement in learning- disabled children (Doctoral dissertation, University of Southern California, 1974) . Dissertation Abstracts International , 1975, 35, 4287A-! (University Micro- films No. 75-1073) McNemar, Q. Psychological statistics (4th ed.). New York: John Wiley & Sons, 1969. Metts, R. E. The Myers-Briggs Type Indicator: Its use with learning disabled adolescents. Doctoral dissertation, Teachers College, Columbia University, 1979. Morse, P. K. Distribution of random response scores on the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator" Paper presented at the First National Conference on the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator, Gainesville, Fla., October 1975. Myers, I. B. Manual: The Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. Palo Alto" Calif. : Consulting Psychologists Press, 1975. (Originally published, 1962.) Myers, I. B. Gifts differing. Palo Alto, Calif.: Consulting Psychologists Press, 1980. Nie, N. H., Hull, C. H., Jenkins, J. G., Steinbrenner , K. , & Bent, D. H. SPSS: Statistical package for the social sciences (2nd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill , T9~75\ Nuernberger , A., & Lawrence, G. Teacher self -understanding . Competency area 1. Module A: Personality influences in teaching and learning . Tallahassee , Fla. : Florida Department of Education, Florida Center for Professional Development Materials, 1974. Plaut, A. Analytical psychologists and psychological types: Comment on replies to a survey. Journal of Analytical Psychology, 17, 2(7), 1972. 129 Quenk, A. T. Psychological types: The auxiliary function and the analytic process. Diploma thesis submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements of the Inter- regional Society of Jungian Analysts. January 1978. Rosenthal, J. H. Self-esteem in dyslexic children. Academic Therapy, 1973, 9, 27-39. Rosser, G. J. A comparative analysis of real-ideal self- concept of non-disabled and language and/or learning disabled children (Doctoral dissertation, Baylor University, 1973) . Dissertation Abstracts Inter- national, 1974, 35, 270A. (University Microfilms No. 74-7294) Rowe, M. B. Who chooses science? A profile. The Science Teacher, 1978, 45(4), 25-28. Siegel, E. The exceptional child grows up. New York: Dut ton , 1975. Thompson, A. Moving toward adulthood. In L. Anderson (Ed.), Helping the adolescent with the hidden handicap. Los Angeles : Academic Therapy Publications, 1970. Van der Hoop, J. H. Conscious orientation: A study of personality types in relation to neurosis and psychosis (Hut ton, L. , Trans.) . New York: Harcourt Brace, 1939. von Franz, M-L., & Hillman, J. Lectures on Jung's typology. New York: Spring Publications, 1971. Wender, P. Minimal brain dysfunction in children. New York: John Wiley, 1971. Weychert, M. C. The effects of similarity-dissimilarity of student-teacher personality type on student attitude and achievement (Doctoral dissertation, Temple Uni- versity, 1975) . Dissertation Abstracts International, 1975, 36(06), 3351A-3352A. (University Microfilms No. 75-28, 258) Wickes, F. G. The inner world of childhood: A study in analytical psychology (Rev"! ed. ) . New York : Apple ton- Century, 1966 . Williams, Jr., D. L. An analysis of the interrelationships among elementary school teachers' personality types, beliefs, observed classroom practices, and reports of how broadcast instructional television should be used (Doctoral dissertation, University of Florida, 1973) . Dissertation Abstracts International, 1973, 34, 3226A. (University Microfilms No. 73-29, 230) 130 Wilmoth, G. H.,& McFarland, S. G. A comparison of four measures of moral reasoning. Journal of Personality- Assessment, 1977, 41(4), 396-40T! BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH Janie Darlene Sweet is the only child of Everett and Selma Kellams, born in Evansville, Indiana, on February 4, 1950. She attended public schools, through tenth grade, in Charlotte, North Carolina. Moving to Los Angeles County, California, in 1966, she graduated from Birmingham High School in the San Fernando Valley in June, 1968. She entered the University of Florida in Fall of 1968 and received her B.A. in psychology in 1972, M.Ed, in psycho- logical foundations of education in 1975, and Ph.D. in foundations of education in 1981. As a graduate student at the University of Florida she served on the staff of the Center for Allied Health Instructional Personnel, under the directorship of Dr. Margaret K. Morgan, as a graduate assistant and graduate research assistant until June, 1975. She then joined the staff of Dr. Mary H. McCaulley at the Center for Applications of Psychological Type, serving as general manager of the center from the time of its establishment to December, 1978. She moved to Chester, South Carolina, in 1979 and completed state requirements for certification as a level II school psychologist in June, 1980, through additional course work at Winthrop College in Rock Hill, South Carolina, and internship to Chester County Schools. She is employed full 131 132 time as a school psychologist for Union County Schools, Union, South Carolina, and will begin the required internship for level III certification in school psychology. Following graduation she will teach part-time at Winthrop College. She has two sons, Scott, age 10 and George, Jr., age 2. Her husband, George Montgomery Sweet, also a UF graduate, is a math instructor at Winthrop College and is working toward completion of his doctoral program in mathematics. Her major interests are family activities, small-scale farming, fishing, and reading. I certify that I have read this study and that in my opinion it conforms to acceptable standards of scholarly presentation and is fully adequate, in scope and quality, as a dissertation for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy. Donald F. Avila, Chairman Professor of Foundations of Education I certify that I have read this study and that in my opinion it conforms to acceptable standards of scholarly presentation and is fully adequate, in scope and quality, as a dissertation for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy. Wilson H. Guertin Professor of Foundations of Education I certify that I have read this study and that in my opinion it conforms to acceptable standards of scholarly presentation and is fully adequate, in scope and quality, as a dissertation for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy. Richard J. Anderson Professor Emeritus, Psychology This dissertation was submitted to the Graduate Faculty of the Department of Foundations of Education in the College of Education and to the Graduate Council, and was accepted as partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy. August 1981 ^fjj^t^^Cuu^ Chairman , Foundations of Education Dean, Graduate School UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA 3 1262 08553 1290