! | Peabody Museum of Natural History Yale University New Haven, CT 06511 Postilla number 1g7 15 February 1982 The Relationships of the Yellow-breasted Chat (/cteria virens) and the Alleged Slowdown in the Rate of Macromolecular Evolution in Birds if RY Charles G. Sibley Jon E. Ahiquist (Received 14 May 1981) Abstract The taxonomic relationships of the Yellow- breasted Chat (/cteria virens) have been Uncertain since its discovery more than 200 years ago. Although usually considered to be a New World wood warbler (Parulini) it possesses structural and behavioral char- acteristics that seem aberrant in compari- Son with the typical members of that group. The relationships of /cteria were inves- tigated by comparing its single-copy DNA Sequences with those of other New World nine-primaried oscines and representatives of other oscine families, using the technique of DNA-DNA hybridization. The data indi- Cate that /cteria is a paruline warbler and 'tshould continue to be included within that group. The study of /eteria provided the basis for an examination of the suggestion by Several authors that the proteins of birds and, by extension, their DNAs, evolve more Slowly than do those of other animals. Evi- dence is presented indicating that the al- leged differences are due, at least in part, to differences in the human perception of the boundaries of taxonomic categories in birds versus most other organisms. Birds are taxonomically oversplit at all supra- Copyright 1982 by the Peabody Museum of Natu- al History, Yale University. All rights reserved. No Part of this publication, except brief quotations for Scholarly purposes, may be reproduced without the Written permission of the Director, Peabody Museum of Natural History. specific levels, but small, nocturnal mam- mals and other groups are probably over- lumped at all levels. The lack of equivalence between the taxonomic categories of birds and those of other animals results in an er- roneous evaluation of their rates of macro- molecular evolution. DNA hybridization data indicate that the vireos (Vireoninae) are not closely related to the wood war- blers, or to other New World nine-primaried oscines. We have shown elsewhere that the vireos are members of a large, varied “corvine assemblage.” Key Words DNA-DNA hybridization, birds, Yellow- breasted Chat, macromolecular evolution, rate slowdown, avian systematics, categori- cal equivalence. Introduction The Yellow-breasted Chat (/cteria virens) breeds from southern Canada and the United States to central Mexico (Jalisco). It has been included among the New World wood warblers (Fringillidae: Emberizinae: Parulini, cf. Sibley, 1970; Sibley and Ahl- quist, in press, a,c) for more than a century but its affinities repeatedly have been ques- tioned because it is aberrant In comparison with the other species assigned to that group. /cteria is larger than any of the typi- cal wood warblers and it differs from them in several structural and behavioral charac- ters. Although it is a New World nine- 2 Yellow-breasted Chat Relationships Postilla 187 primaried oscine, it does not fit readily into any of the subgroups of that large assemblage. Baird (1858) was the first to assign /cte- ria to the wood warblers and (p. 248) he noted the controversy surrounding It. The proper position of this genus has always been a matter of much uncertainty, but | see no reason why it may not legiti- mately be assigned to the Sy/vicolinae, possessing, as it does, so many of their characteristics. The bill is stouter and more curved than in the rest, but the other char- acters agree very well. It cannot properly be placed with the vireos and shrikes on ac- count of the absence of a spurious primary, as well as of a notch in either mandible. But the doubts soon returned. Coues (1892:311) questioned whether /cteria is “most naturally classed with the Warblers” and Newton (1896:85) noted that /cteria “is generally referred to the Family Mniotilti- dae, or American Warblers, but may possi- bly not belong to them, its stout bill being very unlike that possessed by the rest.” However, Baird's opinion was supported by Ridgway (1902:426) who wrote that The position of /eteria in the Mniotiltidae has more than once been questioned; indeed it had not been referred to this family at all until 1858, when Professor Baird formally placed it here as sole repre- sentative of a group or section /cterieae. That he was fully justified in doing so is quite certain, for, however unlike other North American Mniotiltidae /cteria may seem, the extralimital genera Chamaeth- /ypisand Granatellus distinctly connect it with more typical forms, the former being, indeed, a very near relative, its close rela- tionship being shown even in the colora- tion. Bent (1953:587) noted that “Audubon classed [/cteria] with the manakins, and others have placed it with the vireos or with the honeycreepers, but structurally it seems to be most closely related to the wood warblers...” The fifth edition of the American Ornithologists’ Union Check-list (1957) placed /cteria between Chamaeth- /ypisand Euthlypis, without even a foot- note, and its acceptance as a paruline seemed to be settled, but not for long. Eisenmann (1962) reopened the debate by questioning the validity of Chamaeth- /ypis as distinct from Geoth/ypis and sug- gested these two genera be merged. Part of his argument related to Ridgway’s (1902) statement that /cteria was linked to the typical parulines via Chamaethl/ypis and Granatellus. Eisenmann sought, and found, support for the idea that “Recent anatomical studies strongly suggest that /cteria is probably out of place in the wood- warblers.” The evidence cited by Eisenmann included “ /n //tt” communications from W. J. Beecher, who noted that a reexamination of his notes on the jaw musculature of /cte- ria showed that it “could be a tanager’; from William George, who reported that, in certain aspects, the hyoid apparatus of /cte- ria differs markedly” from that of “all conti- nental genera traditionally included in the Parulidae,” as well as from that of “numer- ous genera of tanagers and other Oscines’; and from C. G. Sibley who advised Eisen- mann “that the electrophoretic patterns of the egg-white proteins” of /cteria “are strik- ingly different” from those of the “typical” warblers and tanagers that were examined. Eisenmann’s paper stimulated Ficken and Ficken (1962) to add to the evidence that “the Yellow-breasted Chat is not prop- erly classified as a parulid.” They cited as “aberrant characters” of /cteria its nest structure, eggs, lack of natal down, com- plete post-juvenal molt (“which also occurs in Geothlypis trichas, but not in most other warblers”), color of mouth lining, song char- acteristics, nocturnal singing, courtship dis- play, lack of a distraction display, and the habit of holding food with its feet. They concluded that “the Chat is not a parulid, but that its true relationships remain obscure.” These observations served to reopen the debate about the relationships of /cteria, al- though it now seems clear that the senior author provided Eisenmann with erroneous information in 1962. We do not now recall the basis for the statement quoted by Eisen- 3 Yellow-breasted Chat Relationships Postilla 187 mann (1962) but an examination of the electrophoretic patterns of the groups in question (Sibley, 1970, fig. 28), reveals that those of /cteria match those of the paruline Warblers and other New World nine- primaried oscines, rather than being “strik- ingly different” from them. New data on this problem have been presented by Avise et al. (1980a) from elec- trophoretic comparisons of 16 proteins in 28 species representing 12 genera of paru- line warblers (including /cteria), a thrush (Catharus ustulatus), and a vireo ( Vireo olivaceus). They found that /cter/a, al- though the most distinctive of the wood warblers, is closer to them than to the thrush or the vireo. In this paper we report the results of Comparisons among the homologous nu- cleotide sequences of the single-copy DNAs of /eteria virens and representative genera of the wood warblers (Parulini), the tanagers (Thraupini), the buntings (Emberi- Zini), the New World blackbirds (icterini), the cardueline finches (Carduelini), the Vireos (Vireonidae), the mimic thrushes (Muscicapidae: Mimini) and the wrens (Troglodytidae). These taxonomic alloca- tions follow Sibley (1970) and Sibley and Ahlquist (1980; in press, a,b,c,e,f). In addition, we doubt the interpretation of the evidence that has been presented purporting to show that the proteins, and Presumably also the DNAs, of birds evolve More slowly than do those of mammals and reptiles. Methods The genetic material, DNA (deoxyribonu- Cleic acid), is composed of two linear Chains of four kinds of subunits called nu- Cleotides. The four types of nucleotides differ in the structures of their “bases,” Which are adenine (A), guanine (G), thymine (T), and cytosine (C). In double-stranded DNA the four bases occur as complemen- tary pairs, an adenine in one chain can pair Only with a thymine in the other, a guanine Can pair only with a cytosine. This A-T and G-C base pairing results in the two single strands being complementary nucleotide sequences of one another. Genetic informa- tion is encoded in the sequence of the bases in the DNA strands. In this paper the word “homologous” is used with two meanings. As applied to nu- cleotide sequences it means that two se- quences, or genes, are the descendants of the same sequence In the common ances- tral species. This equals the “orthologous” type of homology of Fitch (1976:161), defined as two different genes “whose dif- ference is a consequence of independence arising from speciation... because there is an exact phyletic correspondence between the history of the genes and the history of the taxa from which they derive.” Homolo- gous, as applied to DNA-DNA hybrids, means a homoduplex hybrid composed of labeled and unlabeled DNA of the same species. Heterologous (or heteroduplex) hy- brids are composed of DNAs from two dif- ferent species. The DNA hybridization technique takes advantage of the complementary structure of the double-stranded DNA molecule. When double-stranded DNA in solution is heated to ca. 100°C the hydrogen bonds be- tween A-T and G-C base pairs dissociate and the two strands separate. Under proper conditions of temperature and salt concen- tration the two single strands will reassoci- ate as the solution cools because the complementary bases “recognize” one another. If the temperature is maintained at a high enough level, e.g., 60°C, complemen- tary base pairing will occur only between long homologous sequences of nucleotides. This is because only long sequences of complementary bases will have sufficient bonding strength to form stable duplexes at that temperature, and only homologous sequences possess the necessary degree of complementarity. Thus, under appropriate conditions of temperature and salt concen- tration, conspecific double-stranded DNA may be thermally dissociated and, because of their inherent properties, the single strands will reassociate only with their homologous partners. 4 Yellow-breasted Chat Relationships Postilla 187 Similarly, if the single-stranded DNAs of two different species are combined under conditions favoring reassociation hybrid double-stranded molecules will form be- tween homologous sequences. These se- quences will contain mismatched bases as a result of the nucleotide sequence dif- ferences that have evolved since the two species diverged from their most recent common ancestor. The lower bonding strength of such hybrid duplexes will cause them to dissociate at a temperature lower than that required to melt conspecific double-stranded DNA. Thus the property of sequence recognition exhibited by homolo- gous sequences and the decreased thermal stability of imperfectly matched hybrid se- quences form the basis of the DNA-DNA hybridization technique. The extent of base pair matching be- tween the homologous nucleotide se- quences of any two DNAs can be deter- mined by measuring (1) the percentage of hybridization and (2) the thermal stability of the reassociated duplex molecules. Follow- ing is a synopsis of the technique which is described in more detail by Sibley and Ahl- quist (1981). Nuclear DNAs from avian erythrocytes were purified (Marmur 1961, Shields and Straus 1975), sheared to an average frag- ment length of ca. 500 nucleotides by soni- cation, and sized by electrophoretic com- parison with DNA fragments of known size produced by the digestion of bacteriophage DNA with bacterial restriction endonuc- leases (Nathans and Smith 1975). Single- copy DNA was prepared consisting of one copy per genome of each single-copy se- quence, plus at /east one copy per genome of each repeated sequence. Such a preparation contains more than 98% of the “complexity” of the genome, i.e., the total length of a/fferent DNA sequences (Britten 1971). Kohne (1970:333-347) discussed the method and reasons for removing the extra copies of repeated sequences in stud- ies designed to determine “the extent of nu- cleotide change since the divergence of two species” (p. 347). We removed the excess repetitive sequences by reassociat- ing the single-stranded DNA of the species to be “labeled” with radioiodine to a Cot value of 1000 at 50°C in 0.48M sodium phosphate buffer (Cot = the initial concen- tration of DNA in moles per liter times the time of incubation in seconds). (Kohne 1970:334). The single-copy sequences were labeled with radioactive iodine ('*°l) according to the procedures of Commorford (1971) and Prensky (1976). DNA-DNA hybrids were formed from a mixture composed of one part (=250 ng) '°l-labeled single-copy DNA and 1000 parts (=250 jg) of sheared, whole DNA at a concentration of 2 mg/ml in 0.48 M sodium phosphate buffer. The hybrid combinations were heated to 100°C for 10 min to dissociate the double- stranded molecules into single strands, then incubated for at least 120 h (=Cot 16,000) at 60°C to permit the single strands to form double-stranded hybrid molecules. The hybrids were bound to hydroxyapa- tite columns immersed in a temperature- controlled water bath at 55°C and the tem- perature was then raised in 2.5°C incre- ments from 55°C to 95°C. At each of the 17 temperatures the single-stranded DNA was eluted in 20 ml of 0.12M sodium phosphate buffer. The radioactivity in each eluted sample was counted in a Packard Model 5220 Auto-Gamma Scintillation Spectrometer, optimized for '°|. A teletype unit connected to the gamma counter printed out the data and punched a paper tape which is the entry to the computer program. The computer program used a nonlinear regression least squares procedure to deter- mine the best fit of the experimental data to one of four functions: 1) the Normal, 2) the dual-Normal, 3) the “skewed” Normal, or 4) a modified form of the Fermi-Dirac equa- tion. The modal temperatures for each hybrid were calculated from the fitted curves. The differences (in °C) between the mode of the homologous hybrid, and that of a heterologous hybrid is the delta mode. 5 Yellow-breasted Chat Relationships Postilla 187 Results Table 1 contains the delta mode values and Figure 1 is adiagram constructed from them. The delta mode values are measure- ments between the labeled species and the other species in Table 1, but not among the other species. However, two species that have the same delta mode value are equidistant from the labeled taxon, but they can be any distance from one another Which is equal to, or less than, their common distance from the labeled species. The data indicate that /cteria is most Closely related to the wood warblers (Geothlypis, Vermivora, Dendroica), and that the tanagers ( Tangara, Ramphocelus), the bunting (Zonotrichia), the grackle (Quiscalus), and the cardueline (Carpo- dacus) are progressively more distant. These genera are members of the Fringilli- dae as defined by Sibley (1970), and Sibley and Ahlquist (in press, a, c, e, f). The vireo, the catbird, and the wren are still more dis- tant from /cteria. Because /cteria is a vocal mimic it has been proposed that it might be related to the mockingbirds (Miminae), but the /cteria:Dumetella delta mode value of 11.0 indicates that the two taxa are as dis- tant from one another as /cteria is from the wrens (11.9) or, as reported by Sibley and Ahiquist (in press, a), as Himatione is from Sturnus, Monarcha, Turdus, Sylvia, Vireo, and Corvus, which differ from Himatione by delta modes from 10.2 to 11.0 (average == HO} 7A). The DNA hybridization data indicate that /cteria is a wood warbler, although it Must represent an early branch in the phy- logeny of the group. Its atypical anatomical and behavioral characters should be Viewed as adaptive specializations, not as evidence that /cteria is more closely related to some group other than the Parulini. The vireos have been thought to be Closely related to the New World nine- Primaried oscines at least since 1930, when Wetmore placed them next to the New World nine-primaried groups. Table 1 Includes a DNA hybrid between /cteria and the Red-eyed Vireo ( Vireo olivaceus) which has a delta mode value of.10.4. Similarly, in our study of the Hawaiian honeycreepers (Fringillidae:Carduelinae: Drepaninini) a DNA hybrid between the Apapane (Himatione sanguinea) and the Red-eyed Vireo had a delta mode of 11.3 (Sibley and Ahiquist, in press, a). These data indicate that Vireo is not closely relat- ed to the New World nine-primaried assem- blage. Avise et al. (1980a) presented evi- dence that Vireo is even more distant from the paruline warblers than are the turdine thrushes, represented by the Swainson’s Thrush (Catharus ustulatus). A DNA hybridization study in which Vireo olivaceus was the radioiodine- labeled taxon has shown that the typical vireos ( Vireo, Hylophilus), the pepper- shrikes (Cyc/arhis) and, presumably, the shrike-vireos ( Vireo/anius) are closely relat- ed to one another, and are not closely relat- ed to the American nine-primaried groups. Instead, they are members of a large, varied, “corvine assemblage” that includes the corvids, monarch flycatchers, cuckoo- shrikes, oriolids, birds-of-paradise, wood swallows, cracticids, drongos, and shrikes. The DNA hybrids between Vireo and mem- bers of these groups have delta values be- tween 7.5 and 9.4 (Sibley and Ahlquist, in press, c). Additional data pertaining to the “corvine assemblage” are included in Sibley and Ahlquist (in press,d, g, h, i). 6 Yellow-breasted Chat Relationships Postilla 187 WY 1 aay 3 e o 5 Wn pe ) : : > - 0 Ay — lomo} = > (el. ao so 36 5 aa S Sy O:= loRe) a, err a Po es gs oS a ° oO ps ~~ ae) ro) wn O jou 0 ~ ete 5 eect eee Soe ieee = tie htt Of ora ae “Sg O So ge Dial 4 ca co) i: 5.3 N=3 5 5) ina? S 6- SS eel n=2 fay) | 7.4n=2 _— — 7 Je) |i ® 8. Le Q Ko) 10.4 n=1 11.4 N=3 Zee 14 Fig. 1 Diagram based on the delta mode values given in Table 1. Because only the DNA of the Yellow- breasted Chat was labeled with radioactive iodine, the diagram depicts only the distances between it and the other taxa and not among those taxa. 7 Yellow-breasted Chat Relationships Postilla 187 Table 1. Modal and delta modal values for DNA-DNA hybrids between the radioiodine (1251)-labeled DNA of the Yellow-breasted Chat and the DNAs of some other passerine birds. COMMON NAME SCIENTIFIC NAME MODE AMODE Yellow-breasted Chat Ieterta virens 3c, O50) Common Yellow-throat Geothlypts trtchas 80.5 B55) Tennessee Warbler Vermtvora peregrina 80.0 SPAA5) Magnolia Warbler Dendrotea magnolia UD 52) 6.0 Scrub Tanager Tangara vitrtolina So 5) Silver-beaked Tanager Ramphocelus carbo Uae 6.6 Song Sparrow Zonotrtchta melodta 78.4 7.4 Common Grackle Qutscalus qutscula LSS Ss Purple Finch Carpodacus purpureus TESS, hs) Red-eyed Vireo Vireo oltvaceus 75.4 10.4 Gray Catbird Dumetella carolinensis 74.8 IEE) Bewick's Wren Thryomanes bewtektt W329 JIS) 8 Yellow-breasted Chat Relationships Postilla 187 Discussion /cteria is but one of many avian genera whose relationships have been unclear. The Wrenthrush, Ze/edonia coronata, was considered to be a turdine thrush until the electrophoretic pattern of its egg white pro- teins revealed its relationship to the wood warblers (Sibley, 1968), and the Swallow- tanager, 7ersina viridis, often placed in a monotypic family, is clearly a somewhat modified tanager (Sibley, 1973). The Hoat- zin (Opisthocomus) was placed in the Gal- liformes because of its superficial similarity to the chachalacas (Orta/is), but its egg white proteins (Sibley and Ahiquist, 1973) revealed its cuculiform affinities. There are many additional examples of morphologi- cally distinctive species of birds which have been viewed as taxonomically distant from their closest relatives. The discrepancy between our perception of taxonomic rela- tionships based upon visible morphological characters, and evidence of relationships derived from comparisons of proteins and/or DNAs, deserves careful scrutiny, for it has important implications for systematic and evolutionary biology. One of the mani- festations of this problem is the debate as to whether or not avian protein molecules evolve more slowly than do those of other animals. One of the most interesting and contro- versial discoveries in recent years has been evidence that the amino acid sequences of proteins and the nucleotide sequences of DNA evolve at remarkably uniform average rates. The concept of the “molecular clock,” first proposed by Zuckerkandl and Pauling (1962), has been discussed by many au- thors, including Fitch (1976), Wilson et al. (1977), and Doolittle (1979). We have found evidence for a uniform average rate of DNA evolution (i.e., nucleotide substitution) in our studies of the ratites (Sibley and Ahl- quist, 1981), the Hawaiian honeycreepers (Sibley and Ahlquist, in press, a), the New Zealand Wrens (Sibley, Williams, and Ahl- quist, in press), the vireos (Sibley and AhI- quist, in press, c) and the Australian fairy- wrens (Sibley and Ahlquist, in press, h). Although there is considerable evidence that DNA and proteins evolve at constant or uniform average rates, several studies have led their authors to suggest that the proteins of birds may evolve more slowly than do those of other animals (Prager et al., 1974; Prager and Wilson, 1975; Barrow- clough and Corbin, 1978; Avise et al., 1980a, b). Prager et al. (1974) first suggested that avian proteins evolve more slowly than do those of other organisms. They used the technique of microcomplement fixation and concluded that the ovotransferrins and serum albumins of birds have evolved more slowly than those of mammals, reptiles, or frogs. The average rate of transferrin evolution in birds was calculated as 1.2 “immunologi- cal distance” (I.D.) units per million years, compared with 2.6 |.D units in mammals and 4.7 in snakes. The authors concluded that the rate of transferrin evolution in mammals is “about twice as great as in birds” and that in snakes it “appears to be nearly 4 times as great as in birds” (p. 253). But the authors did not comment on their evidence showing that snake transferrins apparently evolve 1.8 times as fast as do those of mammals. Thus mammalian trans- ferrins appear to evolve faster than those of birds, but slower than those of snakes. Prager et al. (1974) used the 27 “orders” of birds recognized by some contemporary avian systematists (e.g., Wetmore, 1960) and an estimate of fossil datings to arrive at an average interordinal divergence time of 100 million years (MY; MYA, million years ago). It is not easy to refute some aspects of the study by Prager et al. but we do not be- lieve that the fossil datings are accurate nor that the 27 “orders” are equivalent to one another. From our admittedly preliminary and incomplete DNA comparisons it seems clear that some of these “orders” diverged more recently than 85 MY ago. At least one, the “Apterygiformes” of Wetmore (1960), diverged from the “Casuariiformes” not more than 50 MYA (Sibley and Ahiquist. 1981) and few of the living groups diverged 9 Yellow-breasted Chat Relationships Postilla 187 more than 100 MYA (Sibley and Ahlquist, unpublished). We also doubt that the “orders” of birds, mammals, and reptiles represent equivalent degrees of evolutionary divergence. Instead, we believe that avian orders are excessively “split” and that those of mammals, and per- haps also of reptiles, are overly “lumped.” The lack of equivalence between the boundaries of taxonomic categories within and between birds and mammals is demonstrated by the study of Avise et al. (1980a) who defined the problem by stating that protein evolution in birds appears con- servative relative to that of many inverte- brate and nonavian vertebrate groups. By conservative we mean only that at equiva- lent levels of taxonomic recognition many birds appear to exhibit smaller genetic dis- tances at protein-coding loci than of most other kinds of organisms that have been Surveyed. The reason for this conservative pattern remains unknown. One possibility is that protein evolution is decelerated in birds; the protein “clock” may tick ata slower pace. Barrowclough and Corbin (1978:699, table 5) summarized the data from several Studies and compared the genetic dis- tances (D) of Nei (1972) for Drosophila, fish, salamanders, and mammals with those for birds. For local populations the nonavian average Nei distance (D) was 0.037, for birds 0.003; for subspecies the Values were 0.199 and 0.008; for species 0.609 and 0.100, and for genera 0.783 and 0.213. Thus, compared with the Values for birds, the nonavian distances average nearly 12 times as large for local Populations, 25 times for subspecies, six times for species and 3.7 times for genera. If these differences were due only to a slowdown in the rate of avian protein €volution, these ratios should be equal. Avise et al. (1980a, fig. 1) compared the Nei genetic distances for 27 species Of wood warblers (excluding /cteria), a thrush (Catharus), and a vireo ( Vireo) with those of 14 species of New World Cricetine rodents. For congeneric species the rodents had an average Nei distance of 0.40, the wood warblers 0.056: for dif- ferent genera the rodents averaged 1.256, the wood warblers 0.175. In both of these cases the mammalian distances were more than seven times the corresponding values for the birds. /cteria, with a D value of 0.48 from other parulines is considered to be an aberrant wood warbler, but the grasshopper mice (Onychomys) and the white-footed mice (Peromyscus), at aD of 0.56 from one another are considered to be closely related by mammalian sys- tematists. Thus, on the scale used for the cricetines, there would be only two genera of wood warblers for the 28 spe- cies examined by Avise et al. (1980a), viz., /cteria, plus one other genus for the other 27 species which currently are distributed among 11 genera! The discrepancy is further demonstrat- ed by the Nei distances among 11 species of Dendroica plus those for Wi/son/a, Setophaga, and Sejurus noveboracensis which are less (ca. 0.09 maximum) than the distance (0.11) between two closely related species of mice, Peromyscus maniculatus and P. polionotus. Eleven of the 12 genera of wood warblers examined by Avise et al. (1980a), cluster within a Nei distance of 0.28, which is less than the distance (0.34) between Peromyscus /Jeucopusand P. maniculatus. Avise et al. (1980b) have provided another example by comparing the Nei genetic distance values among 13 species of North American sparrows and finches with those among 13 species of sunfishes (Centrarchidae). The 13 avian species are usually placed in eight genera and two families, Fringillidae and Emberizidae. The 123 sunfish species are divided among six genera. The 13 species of birds have a maxi- mum Nei D value of 0.795 (Carpoda- cus.Calcarius) and among six of the eight genera the maximum D value is 0.327. Species placed in different genera have D distances as low as 0.032 (Ammo- dramus sandwichensis.:Zonotrichia albi- collis). 10 Yellow-breasted Chat Relationships Postilla 187 The D distances among the sunfishes are much larger. The seven species of Lepomis have a maximum D value of ca. 0.70 and the greatest distance between two of the sunfishes is D=ca. 1.7. The smallest D value among the sunfishes is ca. 0.16, between Lepomis marginatus and L. megalotis. The six genera are well separated from one another and all inter- generic D values are 0.7 or larger. These examples illustrate the nature of the evidence. If it is assumed that avian taxonomic categories are equivalent to those of nonavian taxa, it does indeed appear that avian proteins diverge more slowly than do those of other animals. But if this assumption is wrong, the apparent slowdown in the rates of change in avian proteins may be an artifact resulting from the different taxonomic evaluations ap- plied to birds which are the result of cer- tain aspects of their biology in which they differ from many other animals. Birds depend primarily, perhaps entire- ly, upon vision and hearing to identify conspecifics and to determine their sex. As a result, they have evolved plumage colors and structures, behavior patterns, and vocalizations which function as species-specific and sexual-recognition signals. Conversely, most mammals, salamanders, and invertebrates probably utilize the chemical senses, especially ol- faction, for the same purposes. Odors are known to influence reproductive activities in mammals (Bronson, 1974; Stoddart, 1980), and laboratory mice (Mus mus- culus) can distinguish between the odors of conspecific individuals belonging to different histocompatibility groups (Yamazaki et al., 1976, 1979). Because humans, like birds, are visual- auditory animals, we are able to detect the actual signal characters used by birds for species and sexual recognition. To our eyes and ears the species of birds appear distinct, but we often erect genera based upon the secondary sexual characters of males, especially in those groups in which sexual dimorphism is pronounced. Sibley (1957) discussed this problem, cited examples from several groups of birds, and concluded that excessive generic splitting by avian taxonomists “is due to erroneous human evaluation of the taxonomic value of the signal characters.” Some diurnal mammals use visual sig- nals and have evolved visible species specific external characters, but most small mammals are nocturnal and even distantly related species and genera tend to look much alike to our eyes. The New World cricetine rodents (Avise et al., 1980a), although differing widely among themselves on the Nei genetic distance scale (up to D=1.8), are similar in color and general appearence. We perceive them as similar in external morphology and emphasize their similarities by plac- ing them in the same subfamily and in large genera composed of genetically di- verse species. Birds are certainly too finely “split” at the generic and familial levels, but small nocturnal mammals may be too “lumped” at these levels. Some ornithologists have long been aware of these problems which exist even within the Class Aves in which there is a lack of equivalence between passe- rines and nonpasserines in the taxonomic ranking of Supraspecific categories. Scla- ter (1880:345-6) noted that “the Oscines are all very closely related to one another, and, in reality, form little more than one group, equivalent to other so-called fami- lies of birds.” Similarly, Gadow (1891:252) suggested that “strictly speaking, all of the Oscines together are of the rank of one family only.” Lucas (1894) also emphasized that the passerines have usu- ally been split into too many families, and Furbringer (1888) recognized only two families of passerines. However, Sharpe et al. (1877-90) used 29 families for the oscines (including Menuridae and Atri- chornithidae) and more recent classifica- tions divide the same group into 51 (Stre- semann, 1934), 52 (Mayr and Amadon, 1951), or 72 families (Wetmore, 1960). From our preliminary DNA hybridization data we suspect that the oscines (Passeres) are composed of between 10 11 Yellow-breasted Chat Relationships Postilla 187 and 20 clusters that may be approximate- ly equivalent to most of the families of nonpasserines. The range of opinion about the classifi- Cation of the Passeres is exemplified by the cluster of taxa known as the “New World nine-primaried oscines.” Mayr and Amadon (1951) divided them into five families with seven subfamilies, Wetmore (1960) into eight families, and Wolters (1980) used 12 families and 11 subfamilies. We have pro- posed that the same groups should be included in a single family (Fringillidae) with two (or three) subfamilies, and eight tribes (Sibley, 1970:99; Sibley and Ahlquist, in press, a, e, f). Sibley (1970:100) suggested that this “should be accompanied by a correlated reduction in the number of genera to be recognized.” When Martin and Selander (1975), Martin (1980), and Smith and Zimmerman (1976) found biochemical evidence of close rela- tionships they recommended that certain passerine genera should be merged. The number of orders into which birds have been divided has also varied widely. Huxley (1867) used only two orders, four Suborders, and 24 “groups” the latter appar- ently equivalent to superfamilies. Sclater (1880) made 26 orders for living birds but Furbringer (1888) used only seven orders, 21 suborders, 39 “gens,” and 76 families, while Seebohm (1890) divided the birds into six subclasses, 14 orders, and 36 Suborders. Among the current classifications, Mayr and Amadon (1951) used 28 orders and Wetmore (1960) used 27, but Wolters (1975:4) divided the living birds into 49 orders. Again, from our preliminary DNA hy- bridization data, we suspect that living birds can be divided into approximately 20 groups that will merit ordinal rank. The discrepancy between avian and Mammalian orders was apparent to Romer (1962:67) who suggested that “apart from the ratites, most birds... are rather uniform in basic anatomic features, with differences between orders no greater than those which distinguish the smaller groups, termed families, among mammals.” This could also mean that the mammalian orders are overly large. If the number of avian genera is reduced in proportion to the reduction in other categories the discrepancy between avian and mammalian taxa will also be reduced. Bock and Farrand (1980) suggested that the ca. 2945 genera of birds currently recognized could be reduced to ca. 1000. They note that “avian genera are too finely divided and that the genus... has limited meaning in avian classification.” Although birds are apparently too finely divided at the generic, familial, and ordinal levels, there is no reason to believe that they are equally oversplit at the species level. In fact, the human perception of avian species is probably more nearly correct than is our perception of species in many other groups of animals. If this is true we should expect to discover cryptic species in some groups whose true distinctiveness can be detected only by comparisons of their proteins, their DNAs, or their actual species recognition signals —for example, odors or pheromones. This expectation has been realized in a few cases as follows. In an electrophoretic study of 22 protein loci Patton et al. (1976), found that the Heermann Kangaroo Rat (Dipodomys heer- manni) of southern Oregon and California is actually composed of two well-separated species which had been considered con- specific although one has five toes on the hind feet, the other only four. The two spe- cies also differ in the diploid number of chromosomes. Similarly, Highton (1979) discovered a cryptic species of lungless salamander, Plethodon websteri, which is morphologically indistinguishable from P. dorsalis, but electrophoretically different at 80% of 26 genetic loci. The two species are sympatric at one locality in Alabama. Manwell and Baker (1963) discovered a sibling species of sea cucumber (Echinoder- mata: Holothuroidea) when they found two distinct electrophoretic patterns in a popu- lation supposedly representing a single spe- cies. After the two species were character- ized electrophoretically the authors found correlated morphological differences 7 Yellow-breasted Chat Relationships Postilla 187 which had been attributed to individual variation. In birds there are valid species that are difficult to distinguish visually but whose vocalizations are species specific. Examples include the tyrannid genera Empidonax (Stein, 1963; Johnson, 1963), Myiarchus (Lanyon, 1978), and Contopus (Rising and Schueler 1980). Conversely, some avian subspecies are so different in external mor- phological characters that they were long considered to be separate species — for example, the eastern, western, and south- western races of the Common Flicker (Colaptes auratus) (Short, 1966), the eas- tern and western races of the Rufous-sided Towhee (Pipilo erythrophthalmus) (Sibley, 1950; Sibley and West, 1959), and the eastern and western races of the Yellow-rumped Warbler (Dendroica coronata) (Hubbard, 1969; Barrowclough, 1980). It is also clear that closely related species of birds can exist in sympatry without hy- bridizing. The visible and audible species- specific recognition signals of birds are detectable at a considerable distance and function as premating isolating mecha- nisms which prevent pair formation. Evi- dence for this comes from the occasional hybrids between congeners that are widely sympatric but which hybridize only where one of them is uncommon and, therefore, the choice of mates is limited. Examples in- clude the woodpeckers Pico/des pubes- censand P. nutta/lii (Short, 1969) and the bulbuls Pycnonotus caferand P. /eucoge- nys (Sibley and Short, 1959). However, even if hybrids do occur be- tween closely related species of small mammals, salamanders, or other groups of visibly similar species, they would be diffi- cult to detect by the examination of stan- dard museum specimens. The detection of avian hybrids is much easier because the plumage characters of most closely related species are visibly different and the hybrids are distinctive. There are probably other factors perti- nent to this problem but we believe that the evidence for an alternative explanation for the suggested slowdown in the evolution of avian proteins is sufficient to render it doubtful. We suggest that the alleged slow- down is primarily the result of the limita- tions of human perception, not of some un- known difference between the genomes of birds and other animals. The problem of the equivalence of taxonomic categories is not confined to the genera and families of vertebrates. Van Valen (1973) questioned the equivalence of the categories in different phyla and it seems clear that the nonequivalence we have noted among birds, fishes, and mam- mals begins with the lack of equivalence between the groups usually designated as “Classes” in the vertebrates. These are the Agnatha (jawless fishes), Placodermi (jawed, armored fishes), Chondrichthyes (sharks, rays), Osteichthyes (bony fishes), Amphibia, Reptilia, Mammalia, and Aves. These groups are traditionally treated as categorical equivalents, but the Agnatha appear in the fossil record as the jawless os- tracoderms in the Ordovician (ca. 450 MYA), the placoderms, Chondrichthyes and Osteichthyes in the late Silurian or early Devonian (ca. 400 MYA), the Amphib- ia in the late Devonian (ca. 350 MYA), the Reptilia in the Carboniferous (ca. 300 MYA), the Mammalia in the Triassic (ca. 195 MYA), and the Aves in the Jurassic (ca. 130 MYA). The oldest “Class” is nearly three times as old as the youngest. Furthermore, it is apparent that the later groups diverged from the earlier ones and we therefore have Classes evolving from Classes, a logical non sequitur Each so-called Class of verte- brates is subdivided into orders, families, etc., using intraclass characters and the in- evitable result is categorical nonequival- ence throughout the system. The idea that categorical levels might be based upon times of divergence was reject- ed by Simpson (1937) and Mayr (1969:72,230) because it seems apparent that we perceive morphological change as proceeding at many different rates, and there is no way known to quantify the de- grees of difference among morphological characters to reflect degrees of evolutionary iis Yellow-breasted Chat Relationships Postilla 187 divergence. However, Simpson (1944:3) Stated that “Rate of evolution might most desirably be defined as amount of genetic change in a population per year, century, or other unit of absolute time.” But, in 1944, there was no way to measure such a rate of genetic change so Simpson defined the rate of evolution as the “amount of mor- phological change relative to a standard” and assumed that phenotypic evolution im- plies genetic change and that rates of mor- phological evolution “are similar to, al- though not identical with, rates of genetic modification.” This assumption has been implicit (and often explicit) in all morphologically-based classifications of recent years. Unfortunately, it is not true (e.g., Wilson, 1976). Hennig (1966) has been the principal, if not the only, proponent of the age of origin as the basis for the absolute ranking of taxa. He considered the equivalence of ranking to be a serious and important problem, the lack of which is an enormous burden upon systematics that prevents the development of a consistent and maximally useful Classification. Hennig (1966:84, 146, 156, 160) recognized the limitations of morphol- Ogy as the basis for determining the abso- lute rank order of taxonomic groups; so he proposed (pp. 180-182) that the relative rank be determined from morphology and that, where possible, fossil datings and other evidence be used to establish refer- €nce points in the system. He suggested that “there is no single method with which the age of origin can be determined accu- rately” and that only minimal and maximal limits can be recognized. As a compromise, Hennig (p. 191) sug- gested that the present absolute ranking be retained in most groups and that a “conver- Sion chart” be developed to show the €quivalent categories in different groups. Hennig’s reason for favoring such a com- Promise identifies one source of the prob- lem, and what will surely be a barrier to the general acceptance of a time-based ranking of categories. He wrote (p. 191), ... taxonomists are essentially specialists —entomologists, arachnologists, ornitholo- gists, etc.—who... usually work only in cer- tain sections of these extensive areas. All these specialists work as if only their group of animals existed. Consequently each spe- cialist can erect a consistent phylogenetic system for his group without any necessity for correspondence on the basis of equiva- lent age between the absolute rank order of his categories and the absolute rank order of other groups of animals. Presumably even the most convincingly presented ob- jective reasons will not bring these special- ists to the point of giving up life-long habits and speaking of classes and orders where they are accustomed to speaking of families and vice versa. We agree with Hennig that the absolute ranking of taxonomic categories should be based upon the age of origin, and that sister groups should be of equivalent rank. But our reasons for supporting this position are based upon evidence from DNA compari- sons which indicate that the average rate of DNA evolution (i.e., nucleotide substitu- tion) is the same in all lineages. This un/- form average rate of genetic change meets Simpson's (1944:3) criterion for the most desirable definition of the rate of evo- lution and is concordant with Hennig’s arguments in favor of a time-based ranking of taxa. We have presented the arguments and evidence for the uniform average rate else- where (Sibley and Ahlquist, 1981; in press,a; Sibley, Williams, and Ahliquist, in press). The essential points are that DNA hybridization measures the net divergence between the homologous nucleotide se- quences of different taxa and that the uni- form average rate of change is a Statistical result of the large number of nucleotides in the eukaryotic genome, e.g.,ca.2 X 10% in mammals and birds. Each nucleotide evolves at its own rate, and different se- quences evolve at different rates at different times, but when averaged over the genome and over time, the uniform average rate is the inevitable result because there are upper and lower bounds to the rates Yellow-breasted Chat Relationships Postilla 187 and the frequency distribution of rates is narrow relative to the number of nucleo- tides. Thus the rates are not constant, but the average rates in all lineages are uniform. This means that the DNA hybridization data provide the relative time of divergence for any two taxa that are compared. When the DNA values are calibrated against geological or fossil dates the DNA data pro- vide the absolute times of branching and may, therefore, be used to develop a time- based absolute ranking of taxa which Is equivalent to genetic divergence. Because of technical limitations the DNA hybridiza- tion data can be used only for taxa that di- verged during approximately the past 150 MY. However, there are other techniques that can extend the time to the earliest peri- ods of life on Earth. For example, the se- quences of the 16S ribosomal RNAs have been used to determine phylogenetic rela- tionships among bacteria, including diver- gences that occurred as much as three bil- lion years ago (Fox, et al., 1980; Woese, 1981). We therefore propose that the major, and especially the older, dichotomies be dated by the best available fossil and/or nu- cleic acid sequence evidence and that DNA hybridization data be used to develop a genetic divergence-based, and hence time- based, system of taxonomic categories rep- resenting the dichotomies of the last 150 MY. This can largely solve the rank equival- ence problem although, as Hennig so pessimistically predicted, it may take a gen- eration or two of systematists to win acceptance. Acknowledgments For assistance in the laboratory we thank C. Barkan, M. Pitcher, N. Snow, and F.C. Sibley. The computer program was written by TF. Smith. For advice we are indebted to R.J. Britten, D.E. Kohne, R. Holmquist, G.F. Shields, W.F. Thompson, H.E. Burr, and W.M. Fitch. For assistance in the field we thank F.C. Sibley, J. Spendelow, J. O'Neill, R. Semba, J. duPont, and N. and E. Wheelwright. The laboratory work was supported by Yale University and the National Science Foundation (DEB-77-02594 and DEB-79-26746). 15 Yellow-breasted Chat Relationships Postilla 187 Literature Cited American Ornithologist’s Union. 1957. Check-list of North American birds. 5th ed. Am. Ornithol. Union, Lord Baltimore Press, Baltimore, Md. Avise, J.C., J.C. Patton, and C.F. Aquadro. 1980a. Evolutionary genetics of birds. Comparative molecular evolution in New World warblers and rodents. J. Hered. 71:303-310. 1980b. Evolutionary genetics of birds, Il. Conservative protein evolution in North Ameri- can sparrows and relatives. Syst. Zool. 29:323-334. Baird, S.F. 1858. Birds [of the several Pacific railroad routes]. /n U.S. War Dept. Reports of explora- tions and surveys. .. for a railroad from the Mississippi River to the Pacific Ocean, Vol. 9, Part 2. Washington, D.C. Barrowclough, G.F. 1980. Genetic and phenotypic differentiation in a wood warbler (genus Den- droica) hybrid zone. Auk 97:655-668. Barrowclough, G.F. and K.W. Corbin. 1978. Genetic variation and differentiation in the Paruli- dae. Auk 95:691-702. Bent, A.C. 1953. Life histories of North American wood warblers. U.S. Nat. Mus. Bull. 203:1-734. U.S. Government Printing Office, Washington, D.C. Bock, W.J. and J. Farrand, Jr. 1980. The number of species and genera of Recent birds: a contri- bution to comparative systematics. Amer. Mus. Novitates, No. 2703:1-29. Britten, R.J. 1971. Sequence complexity, kinetic complexity, and genetic complexity. Carnegie Inst. Washington Yearb. 69: 503-506. Bronson, F.H. 1974. Pheromonal influences on reproductive activities in rodents. /n M.C. Birch [ed.] Pheromones, Chap. 18, p. 344. Elsevier Excerpta Medica, North Holland Publishing Co. Amsterdam. Commorford, S.L. 1971. lodination of nucleic acids jn vitro. Biochemistry 10:1993-2000. Coues, E. 1892. Key to North American birds. 4th. ed. Estes and Lauriat, Boston. Doolittle, R.F. 1979. Protein evolution. /nH. Neurath and R.L. Hill [eds.] The Proteins, Vol. 4. pp. 1-118. Academic Press, New York. Eisenmann, E. 1962. On the genus “Chamaeth/ypis” and its supposed relationship to /cteria. Auk 79:265-267. Ficken, M.S. and R.W. Ficken. 1962. Some aberrant charcaters of the yellow-breasted chat. Auk 797 182719: Fitch, W.M. 1976. Molecular evolutionary clocks. /n F.J. Ayala [ed.] Molecular Evolution, pp. 160-178. Sinauer Associates, Sunderland, Mass. Fox, G.E., E. Stackebrandt, R.B. Hespell, J. Gibson, J. Maniloff, T.A. Dyer, R.S. Wolfe, W.E. Balch, R.S. Tanner, L.J. Magrum, L.B. Zablen, R. Blakemore, R. Gupta, L. Bonen, B.J. Lewis, D.A. Stahl, K.R. Luehrsen, K.N. Chen, and C.R. Woese. 1980. The phylogeny of prokaryotes. Science 209:457-463. Furbringer, M. 1888. Untersuchungen zur Morphologie und Systematik der Végel Il. Allgemeiner Theil. 875 pp. Van Holkema, Amsterdam. Gadow, H. 1891. Further remarks on the relationships of the Drepanididae. /n S.B. Wilson and Evans [eds.] Aves Hawaiiensis: the birds of the Sandwich Islands, pp. 251-257. R.H. Porter, Ondon. Hennig, W. 1966. Phylogenetic systematics. Univ. Illionis Press, Urbana. 263 pp. Highton, R. 1979. A new cryptic species of salamander of the genus P/ethodon from the south- eastern United States (Amphibia:Plethodontidae). Brimleyana 1:31-36. Hubbard, J.P. 1969. The relationships and evolution of the Dendroica coronata complex. Auk 86:393-432. Huxley, T.H. 1867. On the classification of birds; and on the taxonomic value of the modifica- tions of certain of the cranial bones observable in that class. Proc. Zool. Soc. London 1867:415-472. Johnson, N.K. 1963. Biosystematics of sibling species of flycatchers in the Empidonax hammon- li - oberholseri-wrightii complex. Univ. Calif. Publ. Zool. 66:79-238. Kohne, D.E. 1970. Evolution of higher-organism DNA. ©. Rev. Biophysics 33:327-375. Lanyon, W.E. 1978. Revision of the Myiarchus flycatchers of South America. Amer. Mus. Nat. Hist. Bull. 161:429-627. 16 Yellow-breasted Chat Relationships Postilla 187 Lowery, G.H., Jr. and B.L. Monroe, Jr. 1968. /n R.A. Paynter, Jr. [ed.] Peters Check-list of Birds of the World, Vol. 14, Parulidae, pp.8-93. Mus. Comp. Zool., Cambridge, Mass. Lucas, F.A. 1894. Notes on the anatomy and affinities of the Coerebidae and other American birds. Proc. U.S. Nat. Mus. 17:299-312. Manwell, C. and C.M.A. Baker. 1963. A sibling species of sea cucumber discovered by starch gel electrophoresis. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. 10:39-53. Marmur, J. 1961. A procedure for the isolation of deoxyribonucleic acid from micro-organisms. J. Mol. Biol. 3:208-218. Martin, R.F. 1980. Analysis of hybridization between the hirundinid genera Hirundoand Petro- chelidon in Texas. Auk 97:148-159. Martin, R.F. and R.K. Selander. 1975. Morphological and biochemical evidence of hybridization between cave and barn swallows. Condor 77:362-364. Mayr, E. 1969. Principles of systematic zoology. McGraw-Hill, New York. 428 pp. Mayr, E. and D. Amadon. 1951. A classification of Recent birds. Amer. Mus. Novitates No. 1496:1-42. Mayr, E. and L.L. Short. 1970. Species taxa of North American birds. Publ. Nuttall Ornithol. Club No. 9. Cambridge, Mass. Nathans, D. and H.O. Smith. 1975. Restriction endonucleases in the analysis and restructuring of DNA molecules. Ann. Rev. Biochem. 44:273-293. Nei, M. 1972. Genetic distance between populations. Amer. Nat. 106:283-292. Newton, A. 1896. A dictionary of birds. Part |. (A-AG). Adam and Charles Black, London. Patton, J.L., H. Macarthur, and S.Y. Yang. 1976. Systematic relationships of the four-toed populations of Dipodomys heermanni. J. Mammalogy 57:159-163. Prager, E.M., A.H. Brush, R.A. Nolan, M. Nakanishi, and A.C. Wilson. 1974. Slow evolution of transferrin and albumin in birds according to micro-complement fixation analysis. J. Mol. Evol. 3:243-262. Prager, E.M. and A.C. Wilson. 1975. Slow evolutionary loss of the potential for interspecific hy- bridization in birds: a manifestation of slow regulatory evolution. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. USA. 72:200-204. Prensky, W. 1976. The radioiodination of RNA and DNA to high specific activities. /n D.M. Pres- cott [ed.] Methods in Cell Biology, Vol. 13, pp. 121-152. Academic Press, New York. Ridgway, R. 1902. The birds of North and Middle America, 834 pp. U.S. Nat. Mus. Bull. No. 50, Part 2. U.S. Government Printing Office, Washington, D.C. Rising, J.D. and F.W. Schueler. 1980. Identification and status of wood pewees (Contopus) from the Great Plains: what are sibling species? Condor 82:301-308. Romer, A.S. 1962. The Vertebrate Body. 3rd ed. W.B. Saunders, Philadelphia. Sclater, P.L. 1880. Remarks on the present state of the systema avium. Ibis 22:340-350, 399-411. Seebohm, H. 1890. The classification of birds: an attempt to classify the subclasses, orders, suborders, and some of the families of existing birds. 53 pp. R.H. Porter, London. Sharpe, R.B., H. Seebohm, and P.L. Sclater. 1877-1890. Catalogue of the Passeriformes in the British Museum. Vols. V—XIII. Trustees, Br. Mus., London. Shields, G.F. and N.A. Straus. 1975. DNA-DNA hybridization studies of birds. Evolution 29:159-166. Short, L.L. 1965. Hybridization in the flickers (Co/aptes) of North America. Am. Mus. Nat. Hist. Bull. 129:307-428. 1969. Taxonomic aspects of avian hybridization. Auk 86:84-105. Sibley, C.G. 1950. Species formation in the Red-eyed Towhees of Mexico. Univ. Calif. Publ. Zool. 50:109-194. 1957. The evolutionary and taxonomic significance of sexual dimorphism and hybridiza- tion in birds. Condor 59:166-191. 1968. The relationships of the “wren-thrush," Ze/edonia coronata Ridgway. Postilla (Pea body Mus. Nat. Hist., Yale Univ.) 125:1-12. 1970. A comparative study of the egg-white proteins of passerine birds. Bull. Peabody Mus. Nat. Hist. (Yale Univ.), 32:1-131. 17 Yellow-breasted Chat Relationships Postilla 187 1973. The relationships of the swallow-tanager, 7ersina viridis. Br. Ornithol. Club. Bull. 93:75-79. Sibley, C.G. and J.E. AhIiquist. 1973. The relationships of the Hoatzin. Auk 90:1-13. 1980. The relationships of the “primitive insect eaters” (Aves: Passeriformes) as indicat- ed by DNA-DNA hybridization. Proc. 17th Int. Ornithol. Congr. Berlin, 1978. 1981. The phylogeny and relationships of the ratite birds as indicated by DNA-DNA hy- bridization. /n G.G.E. Scudder and J.L. Reveal [eds.] Evolution Today, Proc. Second Int. Congr. Syst. Evol. Biol., pp. 301-335, Hunt Inst. Bot. Doc., Pittsburgh, Pa. In press, a. The relationships of the Hawaiian honeycreepers (Drepaninini) as indicated by DNA-DNA hybridization. Auk. In press, b. The relationships of the Wrentit (Chamaea fasciata) as indicated by DNA- DNA hybridization. Condor. In press, c. The relationships of the vireos (Vireoninae) as indicated by DNA-DNA hybridi- zation. Wilson Bull. In press, d. The relationships of the Australo-Papuan scrub-robins Drymodes as indicated by DNA-DNA hybridization. Emu. In press, e. The relationships of the wagtails and pipits (Motacillidae) as indicated by DNA-DNA hybridization. L'Oiseau et Rev. Fr. Ornithol. In press, f. The relationships of the Accentors (Prunella) as indicated by DNA-DNA hy- bridization. J. Ornithol. In press, g. The relationships of the Australo-Papuan sittellas Daphoenositta as indicated by DNA-DNA hybridization. Emu. In press, h. The relationships of the Australo-Papuan fairy-wrens as indicated by DNA- DNA hybridization. Emu. In press, i. The relationships of the Australasian whistlers Pachycephalaas indicated by DNA-DNA hybridization. Emu. Sibley, C.G. and L.L. Short, Jr. 1959. Hybridization in some Indian bulbuls Pycnonotus cafer X P. leucogenys. |bis 101:177-182. Sibley, C.G. and D.A. West. 1959. Hybridization in the rufous-sided towhees of the Great Plains. Auk 76:326-338. Sibley, C.G., G.R. Williams, and J.E. AhIquist. In press. The relationships of the New Zealand Wrens (Acanthisittidae) as indicated by DNA-DNA hybridization. Notornis. Simpson, G.G. 1937. Supra-specific variation in nature and in classification from the viewpoint of paleontology. Amer. Nat. 71:236-267. 1944. Tempo and mode in evolution. Columbia Univ. Press. 237 pp. Smith, J.K. and E.G. Zimmerman. 1976. Biochemical genetics and evolution of North American blackbirds, family Icteridae. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. 53B:319-324. Stein, R.C. 1963. |solating mechanisms between populations of Traill’s Flycatchers. Proc. Amer. Phil. Soc. 107:21-50. Stoddart, D.M. ed. 1980. Olfaction in mammals. Zool. Soc. London Symposium No. 45, 364 pp. Academic Press, New York. Stresemann, E. 1934. Aves. /n W. Kiikenthal and T. Krumbach [eds.] Handbuch der Zoologie. Vol. 7, Part 2.899 pp. Walter de Gruyter, Berlin. Van Valen, L. 1973. Are categories in different phyla comparable? Taxon 22:333-374. hata A. 1930. A systematic classification for the birds of the world. Proc. U.S. Nat. Mus. 76 4):1-8. 1960. A classification for the birds of the world. Smithson. Misc. Collec., 139(11) 37 pp. Wilson, A.C. 1976. Gene regulation in evolution. /n F.J. Ayala [ed.] Molecular Evolution, pp. 225-234. Sinauer Assoc., Sunderland, Mass. Wilson, A.C., S.S. Carlson, and T.J. White. 1977. Biochemical evolution. Ann. Rev. Biochem. 46:573-639. Woese, C.R. 1981. Archaebacteria. Sci. Am. 244:98-122. Wolters, H.E. 1975-80. Die Vogelarten der Erde. Parts 1-5. Paul Parey, Hamburg and Berlin. Yellow-breasted Chat Relationships Postilla 187 Yamazaki, K., E.A. Boyse, V. Mike, H.T. Thaler, B.J. Mathieson, J.Abbott, J. Boyse, Z.A. Zayas, and L. Thomas. 1976. Control of mating preferences in mice by genes in the major histo- compatibility complex. J. Exp. Med. 144:1324-1335. Yamazaki, K., M. Yamaguchi, L. Baranoski, J. Bard, E.A. Boyse, and L. Thomas. 1979. Recog- nition among mice. J. Exp. Med. 150:755-760. Zuckerkandl, E. and L. Pauling. 1962. Molecular disease, evolution, and genic diversity. /n M. Kasha and B. Pullman [eds.] Horizons in Biochemistry, pp. 189-225. Academic Press, New York. 19 Yellow-breasted Chat Relationships Postilla 187 The Authors Charles G. Sibley and Jon E. Ahiquist. Department of Biology and Peabody Museum of Natural History, Yale University, 170 Whitney Avenue, P.O. Box 6666, New Haven, CT 06511