• ' '/'.y-. •\v V,-.'...-. •'; Digitized by the Internet Archive in 2010 with funding from Boston Library Consortium IVIember Libraries http://www.archive.org/details/naturalinheritanOOgalt NATURAL INHERITANCE 9- NATURAL INHERITANCE BY FRANCIS GALTON, F.R.S. ^ AUTHOR OF " HEREDITARY GENIUS," " INQUIRIES INTO HUMAN FACULTY," ETC. CHESTNUT liiLL,AIAS& MACMILLAN AND CO. AND LONDON 1894 ITm, Right of Translation and Beprodiiction is Reserved BOSTON OOLLi^vTi^ ..,.x •.>..' ^ CHKST.NIJT BILL, MASb. Kkhard Clav and Sons, Limitkd, LONDON AND BfNGAY. 65941 CONTENTS. CHAPTER I. Introductoky CHAPTER II. Processes in Heredity Natural and Acquired Peculiarities, 4. — Transmutation of Female into Male measures, 5. — Particulate Inheritance, 7. — Family Likeness and Individual Variation, 9. — Latent Characteristics, 11. — Heritages that Blend, and those that are mutually Ex- clusive, 12. — Inheritance of Acquired Faculties, 14.— Variety of Petty Influences, 16. CHAPTER III. Organic Stability 18 Incipient Structure, 18. — Filial Relation, 19. — Stable Forms 20. — Subordinate Positions of Stability, 25. — Model, 27. — Stability of Sports, 30.— Infertility of Mixed Types, 31.— Evolution not by Minute Steps only, 32. CHAPTER IV. Schemes of Distribution and op Frequency 35 Fraternities and Populations to be treated as units, 35. — Schemes of Distribution and their Grades, 37. — The Shape of Schemes is independent of the number of Observations, 44. — Data for eighteen Schemes, 46. — Application of Schemes to inexact measures, 47. — Schemes of Frequency, 49. vi CONTENTS. CHAPTER V. PAGE Normal Variability 51 Scheme of Deviations, 51. — Normal Curve of Distributions, 54. — Comparison of the Observed with the Normal Curve, 56.— The value of a single Deviation at a known Grade, determines a Normal Scheme of Deviations, 60.— Two Measures at known Grades, determine a Normal Scheme of Measures, 61. The Charms of Statistics, 62. — Mechanical Illustration of the Cause of Curve of Frequency, 63.— Order in Apparent Chaos, 66. — Problems in the Law of Error, 66. CHAPTER VI. Data "71 Records of Family Faculties, or R.F.F. data, 72.— Special Data, 78. — Measures at my Anthropometric Laboratory, 79. — Experi- ments in Sweet Peas, 79. CHAPTER VII. Discussion of the Data of Stature 83 Stature as a Subject for Inquiry, 83. — Marriage Selection, 85. — Issue of Unlike Parents, 88, — Description of the Tables of Stature, 91. — Mid-Stature of the Population, 92. — Variability of the Population, 93.' — Variability of Mid-Parents, 93, — Variability in Co-Fraternities, 94. — Regression, — a, Filial, 95 ; &, Mid- Parental, 99 ; c, Parental, 100 ; d, Fraternal, 108.— Squadron of Statures, 110, — Successive Generations of a People, 115. — Natural Selection, 119. — Variability in Frater- nities : First Method, 124 ; Second Method, 127 ; Third Method, 127 ; Fouith Method, 128 ; Trustworthiness of the Constants, 130 ; General view of Kinship, 132. — Separate Con- tribution of each Ancestor, 134. — Pedigree Moths, 136. CHAPTER VIII. Discussion of the Data of Eye Colour 138 Preliminary Remarks, 138, — Data, 139.— Persistence of Eye Colour in the Population, 140. — Fundamental E3'-e Colours, 142. — Principles of Calculation, 148. — Results, 152. CONTENTS. vii CHAPTER IX. PAGE The Artistic Faculty 154 Data, 154. — Sexual Distribution, 156.— Marriage Selection, 157. — Regression, 158. — Effect of Bias in Marriage, 162. CHAPTER X. Disease 164 Preliminary Problem, 165. — Data, 167. — Trustwortbiness of R.F.F. data, 167. — Mixture of Inberitances, 167. — Consump- tion : General remarks, 171 ; Distribution of Fraternities, 174 ; Severely Tainted Fraternities, 176 ; Consumptivity, 181. — Data for Hereditary Diseases, 185. CHAPTER XI. Latent Elements 187 Latent Elements not very numerous, 187 ; Pure Breed, 189. — Simplification of Hereditary Inquiry, 190. CHAPTER XII. Summary , . . . 192 viii CONTENTS. TABLES. The words by which the various Tables are here described, have been chosen for the sake of qnick reference ; they are often not identical with those used in their actual headings. No. of the Subjects OF THE Tables. ' page i abies. 1. Strengths of Pull arranged for drawing a Scheme 199 2. Data for Schemes of Distribution for various Qualities and Faculties 200 3. Evidences of the general applicability of the Law of Frequency of Error 201 4. Values for the Normal Curve of Frequency (extracted from the well-known Table) 202 5. Values of the Probability Integral (extracted from the well- known Table) . . . ." 202 6. A^alues of the Probability Integral when the scale by which the Errors are measured has the Prob : Error for its Unit . 203 7. Ordinates to the Normal Curve of Distribution, when its 100 Grades run from - 50°, through 0°, to -f 50^ 204 8. Ditto when the Grades run from 0° to lOO"". {This Table is espeiially adapted for use with Schemes) 205 9. Marriage Selection in respect to Stature 206 9a. Marriage Selection in respect to Eye Colour 206 9Z), Marriages of the Artistic and the Non Artistic 207 10. Issue of Parents who are unlike in Stature 207 11. Statures of adult children born to Mid Parents of Various Statures 208 1 2. Statures of the Brothers of men of various Statures, fi'om P.F.F. data 209 CONTENTS. ix Tables ^"^ Subjects of the Tables. page 13. Ditto from the Special data 210 14. Deviations of Individual brothers from their common Mid- Fraternal Stature 211 15. Frequency of the different Eye Colours in 4 successive Gene- rations 212 16. The Descent of Hazel-Eyed families 213 17. Calculated contributions from Parents and from Grandparents, according as they are Light, Hazel, or Dark eyed . . . . 213 18. Examples of the application of Table 17 214 19. _ Observed and calculated Eye Colours in 16 groups of families 215 20. Ditto in 78 separate families 216 21. Amounts of error in the various calculations of anticipated Eye Colour 218 22. Inheritance of the Artistic Faculty 218 APPENDICES. A. Memoirs and Books on Heredity by the Author 219 B. Problems by J. D. Hamilton Dickson 221 C. Experiments on Sweet Peas, bearing on the law of Eegression . . 225 D. Good and bad Temper in English Families 226 E. The Geometric Mean in "Vital and Social Statistics . 238 F. Probable extinction of Families, 214 ; Discussion of the Problem by the Eev. H. W. Watson, D.Sc 241 G. Orderly arrangement of Hereditary Data 248 Index 257 NATURAL INHERITANCE NATUKAL INHERITANCE, CHAPTEE L INTRODUCTOEY. I HAVE long been engaged upon certain problems tliat lie at the base of tlie science of heredity, and during several years have published technical memoirs concern- ing them, a list of which is given in Appendix A. This volume contains the more important of the results, set forth in an orderly way, w^ith more completeness than has hitherto been possible, together with a large amount of new matter. The inquiry relates to the inheritance of moderately exceptional qualities by brotherhoods and multitudes rather than by individuals, and it is carried on by more refined and searching methods than those usually employed in hereditary inquiries. One of the problems to be dealt with refers to the curious regularity commonly observed in the statistical peculiarities of great populations during a long series of B 2 NATURAL INHEHITANCE. [chap. generations. The large do not always begefc the large, nor the small the small, and yet the observed propor- tions between the large and the small in each degree of size and in every quality, hardly varies from one gener- ation to another. A second problem regards the average share con- tributed to the personal features of the offspring by each ancestor severally. Though one half of every child may be said to be derived from either parent, yet he may receive a heritage from a distant progenitor that neither of his parents possessed as personal character- istics. Therefore the child does not on the average receive so much as one half of his personal qualities from each parent, but something less than a half. The question I have to solve, in a reasonable and not merely in a statistical way, is, how much less ? The last of the problems that I need mention now, concerns the nearness of kinship in different degrees. We are all ag;reed that a brother is nearer akin than a nephew, and a nephew than a cousin, and so on, but how much nearer are they in the precise language of numerical statement ? These and many other problems are all fundamentally connected, and I have worked them out to a first degree of approximation, with some completeness. The con- clusions cannot however be intelligibly presented in an introductory chapter. They depend on ideas that must first be well comprehended, and which are now novel to the large majority of readers and unfamiliar to all. But those who care to brace themselves to a I.] INTRODUCTORY. 3 sustained effort, need not feel much regret that the road to be travelled over is indirect, and does not admit of being ma^pped beforehand in a way they can clearly understand. It is full of interest of its own. It familiarizes us with the measurement of variability, and with curious laws of chance that apply to a vast diversity of social subjects. This part of the inquiry may be said to run along a road on a high level, that affords wide views in unexpected directions, and from w^hich easy descents may be made to totally different goals to those we have now to reach. I have a great subject to write upon, but feel keenly my literary incapacity to make it easily intelligible without sacrificing accuracy and thoroughness. r P> 2 r CHAPTEE 11. PROCESSES m HEREDITY. Natural and Acquired Peculiarities. — Transmutation of Female into Male Measures. — Particulate Inheritance. — Family Likeness and Individual Variation. — Latent Characteristics. — Heritages tliat Blend and those that are Mutually Exclusive. — Inheritance of Acquired Faculties. — Variety of Petty Influences. A CONCISE account of the cliief processes in heredity will be given in this chapter, partly to serve as a reminder to those to whom the works of Darwdn especi- ally, and of other writers on the subject, are not familiar, but principally for the sake of presenting them under an aspect that best justifies the methods of investigation about to be employed. Natural and Acquired Peculiarities. — The peculiari- ties of men may be roughly sorted into those that are natural and those that are acquired. It is of the former that I am about to s|)eak in this book. They are noticeable in every direction, but are nowhere so remarkable as in those twins ^ who have been dissimilar 1 See Human Faculty, 237. CHAP. II.] PROCESSES IN HEIIEDITY. . 5 in features and disposition from tlieir earliest years, thongli brouo^lit into tlie world under the same condi- tions and subsequently nurtured in an almost identical manner. It may be that some natural peculiarity does not appear till late in life, and yet may justly deserve to be considered natural, for if it is decidedly exceptional in its character its origin could hardly be ascribed to the effects of nurture. If it was also possessed by some ancestor, it must be considered to be hereditary as well. But "Natural" is an unfortunate word for our purpose ; it implies that the moment of birth is the earliest date from which the effects of surrounding conditions are to be reckoned, although nurture begins much earlier than that. I therefore must ask that the word "Natural" should not be construed too literally, any more than the analogous phrases of inborn, con- genital, and innate. This convenient laxity of expres- sion for the sake of avoiding a pedantic periphrase need not be accompanied by any laxity of idea. Transmutation of Female into Male Measures. — We shall have to deal with the hereditary influence of parents over their offspring, although the characteristics of the two sexes are so different that it may seem impossible to speak of both in the same terms. The phrase of " Average Stature " may be applied to two men without fear of mistake in its interpretation ; neither can there be any mistake when it is applied to two women, but what meanino^ can we attach to the word " Averas^e " w^hen it is applied to the stature of two such different G NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. beings as the Father and the Mother ? How can we appraise the hereditary contributions of different an- cestors whether in this or in any other quality, unless we take into account the sex of each ancestor, in addi- tion to his or her characteristics ? Again, the same group of progenitors transmits qualities in different measure to the sons and to the daughters ; the sons being on the whole, by virtue of their sex, stronger, taller, hardier, less emotional, and so forth, than the daughters. A serious complexity due to sexual differ- ences seems to await us at every step when investigating the problems of heredity. Fortunately we are able to evade it altogether by using an artifice at the outset, else, looking back as I now can, from the stage which the reader will reach when he finishes this book, I hardly know how we should have succeeded in making a fair start. The artifice is never to deal with female measures as they are observed, but always to employ their male equivalents in the place of them. I trans- mute all the observations of females before taking them in hand, and thenceforward am able to deal with them on equal terms with the observed male values. For example : the statures of women bear to those of men the proportion of about twelve to thir- teen. Consequently by adding to each observed female stature at the rate of one inch for every foot, we are enabled to compare their statures so increased and trans- muted, with the observed statures of males, on equal terms. If the observed stature of a woman is 5 feet, it will count by this rule as 5 feet -\- 5 inches; if it be II.] PROCESSES IN HEIIEDITY. , 7 6 feet, as 6 feet + 6 inclies ; if 5-| feet, as 5^ feet + 5^ inches ; that is to say, as 5 feet + Hi inches/ Simihxiiy as regards sons and daughters ; whatever may be observed or concluded concerning daughters will, if transmuted, be held true as regarding sons, and whatever is said concerning sons, will if re- transrauted, be held true for daughters. We shall see further on that it is easy to apply this principle to all measurable qualities. Particulate Inheritance. — All livino^ beino\s are indi- viduals in one aspect and composite in another. They are stable fabrics of an inconceivably large number of cells, each of which has in some sense a separate life of its own, ancl which have been combined under influences tliat are the subjects of much speculation, but are as yet little understood. We seem to inherit bit by bit, this element from one progenitor that from another, under conditions that will be more clearly expressed as we proceed, \vhile the several bits are themselves liable to some small change during the process of transmission. Inheritance may therefore be described as largely if not wholly " particulate," and as such it will be treated in these pages. Though this word is good English and accurately expresses its own meaning, the application 1 .The proportion I use is as 100 to 108 ; that is, I multiply every female measure by 108, which is a very easy operation to those who possess that most iisefiil book to statisticians, Crelle's Tables (G. Reimer, Berlin, 1875). It gives the products of all numbers under 1000, each into each ; so by referring to the column headed 108 the transmuted values of the female statures can be read off at once. 8 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. now made of it will be better understood through an illus- tration. Thus, many of the modern buildings in Italy are historically knovv^n to have been built out of the pillaged structures of older days. Here we may observe a column or a lintel serving the same purpose for a second time, and perhaps bearing an inscription that testifies to its origin, while as to the other stones, though the mason may have chipped them here and there, and altered their shapes a little, few, if any, came direct from the quarry. This simile gives a rude though true idea of the exact meaning of Particulate Inheritance, namely, that each piece of the new structure is derived from a corresponding piece of some older one, as a lintel was derived from a lintel^ a column from a column, a piece of wall from a piece of wall. I will pursue this rough simile just one step further, which is as much as it will bear. Suppose we were building a house with seeond-hand materials carted from a dealer's yard, we should often find considerable portions of the same old houses to be still grouped together. Materials derived from various structures mio^ht have been moved and much shufiled together in the yard, yet pieces from the same source would frequently remain in juxtaposition and it may be entangled They would lie side by side ready to be carted away at the same time and to be re-erected together anew. So in the process of transmission by inheritance, elements derived from the same ancestor are apt to appear in large groups, just as if they had clung together in the pre-embryonic stage, as perliaps II.] PROCESSES IN HEREDITY. 9 they did. They form what is well expressed by the word " traits," traits of feature and character — that is to say, continuous features and not isolated points. We appear, then, to be severally built up out of a host of minute particles of whose nature we know nothing, any one of which may be derived from any one progenitor, but which are usually transmitted in aggregates, considerable groups being derived from the same progenitor. It would seem that while the embryo is developing itself, the particles more or less qualified for each new post wait as it were in com- petition, to obtain it. Also that the particle that succeeds, must owe its success partly to accident of position and partly to being better qualified than any equally well placed competitor to gain a lodgment. Thus the step by step development of the embryo cannot fail to be influenced by an incalculable number of small and mostly unknown circumstances. Family Likeness and Individual Variation. — Natural peculiarities are apparently due to two broadly difi'erent causes, the one is Family Likeness and the other is In- dividual Variation. They seem to be fundamentally opposed, and to require independent discussion, but this is not the case altogether, nor indeed in the greater part. It will soon be understood how the conditions that pro- duce a general resemblance between the offspring and their parents, must at the same time give rise to a con- siderable amount of individual differences. Therefore I need not discuss Family Likeness and Individual Varia- 10 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. tion under separate lieads, but as different effects of the same underlying causes. The origin of these and other prominent processes in heredity is best exphiined by illustrations. That which will be used was suggested by those miniature gardens, self-made and self-sown, that may be seen in crevices or other receptacles for drifted earth, on the otherwise bare faces of quarries and cliffs. I have frequently studied them through an opera glass, and have occasionally clambered up to compare more closely their respective vegetations. Let us then suppose the aspect of the vegetation, not of one of these detached little gardens, but of a particular island of substantial size, to represent the features, bodily and mental, of some particular parent. Imagine two such islands floated far away to a desolate sea, and anchored near together, to represent the two parents. Next imagine a number of islets, each constructed of earth that was wholly destitute of seeds, to be reared near to them. Seeds from both of the islands will gradually make their way to the islets through the agenc}^ of winds, currents, and birds. Vegetation will spring up, and when the islets are covered with it, their several aspects will represent the features of the several children. It is almost impossible that the seeds could ever be distributed equally among the islets, and there must be slight differences between them in exposure and other conditions, corresponding to differences in pre-natal circumstances. All of these would have some influence upon the vegetation ; hence there would be a corre- II.] PROCESSES IN HEREDITY. 11 spoiiding variety in the results. In some islets one plant would prevail, in others another ; nevertheless there would be many traits of family likeness in the vegetation of all of them, and no plant would be found that had not existed in one or other of the islands. Though family likeness and individual variations are largely due to a common cause, some variations are so large and otherwise remarkable, that they seem to belong to a different class. They are known among breeders as " sports " ; I will speak of these later on. Latent Characteristics. — Another fact in heredity may also be illustrated by the islands and islets ; namely, that the child often resembles an ancestor in some feature or character that neither of his parents personally possessed. AVe are told that buried seeds may lie dormant for many years, so that when a plot of ground that was formerly cultivated is again deeply dug into and upturned, plants that had not been known to grow on the spot within the memory of man, will frequently make their appearance. It is easy to imagine that some of these dormant seeds should find their way to an islet, through currents that undermined the island cliffs and drifted away their dehris, after the cliffs had tumbled into the sea. x\gain, many plants on the islands may maintain an obscure existence, being hidden and half smothered by successful rivals ; but whenever their seeds happened to find their way to any one of the islets, while those of their rivals did not, they would sprout freely and assert themselves. This 12 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. illustration partly covers the analogous fact of diseases and otlier inheritances skipping a generation, which by the way I find to be by no means so usual an occurrence as seems popularly to be imagined. Heritages that Blend and those that are Mutually Exclusive. — As regards heritages that blend in the offspring, let us take the case of human skin colour. The children of the white and the negro are of a blended tint ; they are neither wholly white nor wdioUy black, neither are they piebald, but of a fairly uniform mulatto brown. The quadroon child of the mulatto and the w^hite has a quarter tint ; some of the children ma}' be altogether darker or lighter than the rest, but they are not j)iebald. Skin-colour is therefore a good example of wdiat I call blended in- heritance. It need be none the less '' particulate " in its origin, but the result may be regarded as a fine mosaic too minute for its elements to be distinguished in a oreneral view. Next as regards heritages that come altogether from one progenitor to the exclusion of the rest. Eye-colour is a fairly good illustration of this, the children of a light- eyed and of a dark-eyed parent being much more apt to take their eye-colours after the one or the other than to have intermediate and blended tints. There are probably no heritages that perfectly blend or that absolutely exclude one another, but all heritages have a tendency in one or the other direction, and the tendency is often a very strong one. This is paralleled II.] PROCESSES IN HEREDITY. 13 by what we may see in plots of w^ild vegetation, where two varieties of a plant mix freely, and the general aspect of the vegetation becomes a blend of the two, or where individuals of one variety congregate and take exclusive possession of one place, and those of another variety congregate in another. A peculiar interest attaches itself to mutually exclu- sive heritages, owing to the aid they must afford to the establishment of incipient races. A solitary peculiarity that blended freely with the characteristics of the parent stock, would disappear in hereditary transmission, as quickly as the white tint imported by a solitary Euro- pean would disappear in a black population. If the European mated at all, his spouse must be black, and therefore in the very first generation the offspring would be mulattoes, and half of his whiteness would be lost to them. If these mulattoes did not inter- breed, the whiteness would be reduced in the second generation to one quarter ; in a very few more genera- tions all recognizable trace of it would have gone. But if the whiteness refused to blend with the black- ness, some of the offspring of the wdiite man would be wholly white and the rest wholly black. The same event would occur in the grandchildren, mostly but not exclusively in the children of the white offspring, and so on in subsequent generations. Therefore, unless the white stock became wholly extinct, some undiluted specimens of it w^ould make their appear- ance during an indefinite time, giving it repeated r 14 NATURAL INHEHITANCE. [chap. cliaiices of holding its own in tlie struggle for existence, and of establishing itself if its qualities were superior to those of the black stock under any one of manj different conditions. Inheritance of Acquired Faculties. — I am unpre- pared to say more than a few words on the obscure, unsettled, and much discussed subject of the possibility of transmitting acquired faculties. The main evidence in its favour is the gradual change of the instincts of races at large, in conformity with changed habits, and through their increased adaptation to their surroundings, otherwise apparently than through the influence of Natural Selection. There is very little direct evidence of its influence in the course of a single generation, if the phrase of Accjuired Faculties is used in perfect strictness and all inheritance is excluded that could bo referred to some form of Natural Selection, or of Infection before birth, or of peculiarities of Nurture and Eearing. Moreover, a large deduction from the collection of rare cases must be made on the ground of their being accidental coincidences. When this is done, the remaining instances of acquired disease or faculty, or of any mutilation being transmitted from parent to child, are very few. Some apparent evidence of a j)ositive kind, that was formerly relied upon, has been since found capable of being interpreted in another way, and is no longer adduced. On the other hand there exists such a vast mass of distinctly negative evidence, that every instance offered to prove the transmission II.] PROCESSES IN HEREDITY. 15 of acquired faculties requires to be closely criticized. For example, a woman wlio was sober becomes a drunkard. Her children born during the period of her sobriety are said to be. quite healthy ; her subsequent chil- dren are said to be neurotic. The objections to accepting this as a valid instance in point are many. The woman's tissues must have been drenched with alcohol, and the unborn infant alcoholised during all its existence in that state. The quality of the mother's milk would be bad. The surroundings of a home under the charge of a drunken woman would be prejudicial to the health of a growing child. No wonder that it became neurotic. Again, a large number of diseases are conveyed by germs capable of passing from the tissues of the mother into those of the unborn child otherwise than through the blood. Moreover it must be recollected that the connection between the unborn child and the mother is hardly more intimate than that between some parasites and the animals on which they live. Not a single nerve has been traced between them, not a drop of blood ^ has been found to pass from the mother to the child. The unborn child together with the growth to which it is attached, and which is afterwards thrown off, have their own vascular system to them- selves, entirely independent of that of the mother. If in an anatomical preparation the veins of the mother are injected with a coloured fluid, none of it enters the veins of the child ; conversely, if the veins of the child 1 See Lectures by William 0. Priestley, M.D. (CluircMll, London, 1860), pp. 50, 52, 55, 59, and 64. 16 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. are injected, none of the fluid enters those of the mother. Again, not only is the unborn child a sepa- rate animal from its mother, that obtains its air and nourishment from her purely through soakage, but its constituent elements are of very much less recent growth than is popularly supposed. The ovary of the mother is as old as the mother herself; it was well developed in her own embryonic state. The ova it con- tains in her adult life were actually or potentially present before she was born, and they grew as she grew. There is more reason to look on them as collateral with the mother, than as parts of the mother. The same may be said with little reservation concerning the male elements. It is therefore extremely difficult to see how acquired faculties can be inherited by the children. It would be less difficult to conceive of their inheritance by the grandchildren. Well devised experiment into the limits of the power of inheriting acquired faculties and mutilations, whether in plants or animals, is one of the present desiderata in hereditary science. Fortunately for us, our ignorance of the subject will not introduce any special difficulty in the inquiry on which we are now engaged. Variety of Petty Influences.— The mcalculshle number of petty accidents that concur to produce variability among brothers, make it impossible to predict the exact cjualities of any individual from hereditary data. But we may predict average results with great cer- tainty, as will be seen further on, and we can also II.] PROCESSES IN HEREDITY. 17 obtain precise information concerning the penumbra of uncertainty that attaches itself to single predic- tions. It would be premature to speak further of this at present ; what has been said is enough to give a clue to the chief motive of this chapter. Its intention has been to show the large part that is always played by chance in the course of hereditary transmission , and to establish the importance of an intelligent use of the laws of chance and of the statistical methods that are based upon them, in expressing the conditions under which heredity acts. I may here point out that, as the processes of statis- tics are themselves processes of intimate blendings, their results are the same, whether the materials had been partially blended or not, before they were statistically taken in hand. CHAPTER III. ORGIANIC STABILITY. Incipient Structure. — Filial relation. — Stable Forms. — Subordinate posi- tions of Stability. — Model. — Stability of Sports. — Infertility of mixed Types. — Evolution not by minute steps only. Incipient Structure. — The total heritage of each man must include a greater variety of material than was utilised informing his personal structure. The existence in some latent form of an unused portion is proved by his power, already alluded to, of transmitting ancestral characters that he did not personally exhibit. There- fore the organised structure of each individual should be viewed as the fulfilment of only one out of an indefinite number of mutually exclusive possibilities. His struc- ture is the coherent and more or less stable development of what is no more than an imperfect sample of a large variety of elements. The precise conditions under which each several element or particle (whatever may be its nature) finds its way into the sample are, it is needless to repeat, unknown, but we may provisionally classify them under one or other of the following three categories, as they CHAP. III.] ORGANIC STABILITY. 19 apparently exhaust all reasonable possibilities : first, that in which each element selects its most suitable immediate neighbourhood, in accordance with the guiding idea in Darwin's theory of Pangenesis ; secondly, that of more or less general co-ordination of the influences exerted on each element, not only by its immediate neighbours, but by many or most of the others as well ; finally, that of accident or chance, under which name a group of agen- cies are to be comprehended, diverse in character and alike only in the fact that their influence on the settlement of each particle was not immediately directed towards that end. In philosophical language we say that such agencies are not purposive, or that they are not teleological ; in popular language they are called accidents or chances. Filial Relation. — A conviction that inheritance is mainly particulate and much influenced by chance, greatly afiects our idea of kinship and makes us con- sider the parental and filial relation to be curiously circuitous. It appears that there is no direct hereditary relation between the personal parents and the personal child, except perhaps through little-known channels of secondary importance, but that the main line of hereditary connection unites the sets of elements out of which the personal parents had been evolved with the set out of which the personal child was evolved. The main line may be rudely likened to the chain of a necklace, and the personalities to pendants attached to its links. We are unable to see the particles and C 2 20 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. watch their grouping, and we know nothing directly about them, but we may gain some idea of the various possible results by noting the differences between the brothers in any large fraternity (as will be done further on with much minuteness), whose total heritages must have been much alike, but whose personal structures are often very dissimilar. This is why it is so im- portant in hereditary inquiry to deal with fraternities rather than with individuals, and with large fraternities rather than small ones. We ought, for example, to compare the group containing both parents and all the uncles and aunts, with that containing all the children. The relative weight to be assigned to the uncles and aunts is a question of detail to be discussed in its proper place further on (see Chap. XL) Stable Forms. — The changes in the substance of the newly-fertilised ova of all animals, of which more is annually becoming known, ^ indicate segregations as well as aggregations, and it is reasonable to suppose that repulsions concur with affinities in producing them. We know nothing as yet of the nature of these affinities and repulsions, but we may expect them to act in great numbers and on all sides in a space of three dimensions, just as the personal likings and dis- 1 A valuable memoir on the state of our knowledge of these matters up to the end of 1887 is published in Vol. XIX. of the Proceedings of the Philosophical Society of Glasgow, and reprinted under the title of Tlie Modern Cell Theory, and Theories as to the Physiological Basis of Heredity^ by Prof. John Gray McKendrick, M.D., F.R.S., &c. (R. Anderson, Glasgow, 1888.) III.] ORGANIC STABILITY. 21 likings of eaeh indiyidual insect in a flying swarm may be supposed to determine the position that he occupies in it. Every particle must have many immediate neigh- bours. Even a sphere surrounded by other spheres of equal sizes, like a cannon-ball in the middle of a heap, when they are piled in the most compact form, is in actual contact with no less than twelve others. We may therefore feel assured that the particles which are still unfixed must be affected by very numerous influences acting from all sides and varying with slight changes of place, and that they may occupy many positions of tem- porary and unsteady equilibrium, and be subject to repeated unsettlement, before they finally assume the positions in which they severally remain at rest. The whimsical eff'ects of chance in producing stable results are common enough. Tangled strings variously twitched, soon get themselves into tight knots. Eub- bish thrown down a sink is pretty sure in time to choke the pipe ; no one bit may be so large as its bore, but several bits in their numerous chance encounters will at length so come into collision as to wedge themselves into a sort of arch across the tube, and efi'ectually plug it. Many years ago there was a fall of large stones from the ruinous walls of Kenilworth Castle. Three of them, if I recollect rightly, or possibly four, fell into a very peculiar arrangement, and bridged the interval between the jambs of an old window. There they stuck fast, showing clearly against the sky. The oddity of the structure attracted continual attention, and its stability was much commented on. These hanging stones, as 22 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. they were called, remained quite firm for many years ; at length a storm shook them down. In every congregation of mutually reacting elements, some characteristic groupings are usually recognised that have become familiar through their frequent re- currence and partial persistence. Being less evanescent than other combinations, they may be regarded as temporarily Stable Forms. No demonstration is needed to show that their number must be greatly smaller than that of all the possible combinations of the same elements. I will briefly give as great a diversity of instances as I can think of, taken from Grovernments, Crowds, Landscapes, and even from Cookery, and shall afterwards draw some illustrations from Mechanical Inventions, to illustrate what is meant by characteristic and stable groupings. From some of them it wiU also be gathered that secondary and other orders of stability exist besides the primary ones. In Governments, the primary varieties of stable forms are very few in number, being such as autocracies, con- stitutional monarchies, oligarchies, or republics. The secondary forms are far more numerous ; still it is hard to meet with an instance of one that cannot be pretty closely paralleled by another. A curious evidence of the small variety of possible governments is to be found in the constitutions of the governing bodies of the Scientific Societies of London and the Provinces, which are numerous and independent. Their development seems to follow a single course that has many stages. III.] ORGANIC STABILITY. 23 and invariably tends to establish tbe following staff of officers : President, vice-Presidents, a Council, Honorary Secretaries, a paid Secretary, Trustees, and a Treasurer. As Britons are not unfrequently servile to rank, some seek a purely ornamental Patron as well. Every variety of Crowd has its own characteristic features. At a national pageant, an evening party, a race-course, a marriage, or a funeral, the groupings in each case recur so habitually that it sometimes appears to me as if time had no existence, and that the ceremony in which I am taking part is identical with others at which I had been present one year, ten years, twenty years, or any other time ago. The frequent combination of the same features in Landscape Scenery, justifies the use of such expressions as " true to nature," when applied to a pictorial com- position or to the descriptions of a novel writer. The experiences of travel in one part of the world may curiously resemble those in another. Thus the military expedition by boats up the Nile was planned from experiences gained on the Eed Eiver of North America, and was carried out with, the aid of Canadian voyageurs. The snow mountains all over the world present the same peculiar difficulties to the climber, so that Swiss experiences and in many cases Swiss guides have been used for the exploration of the Himalayas, the Caucasus, the lofty mountains of New Zealand, the Andes, and Greenland. Whenever the general conditions of a new country resemble our own, we recognise character- istic and familiar features at every turn, whether we 24 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. are walking by the brookside, along the seashore, in the woods, or on the hills. Even in Cookery it seems difficult to invent a new and good dish, though the current recipes are few, and the proportions of the flour, sugar, butter, eggs, &c., used in making them might be indefinitely varied and be still eatable. I consulted cookery books to learn the facts authoritatively, and found the following passage : " I have constantly kept in view the leading principles of this work, namely, to give in these domestic recipes the most exact quantities. ... I maintain that one cannot be too careful ; it is the only way to put an end to those approximations and doubts which will beset the steps of the inexperienced, and which account for so many people eating indifierent meals at home." ^ It is the triteness of these experiences that makes the most varied life monotonous after a time, and many old men as well as Solomon have frequent occasion to lament that there is nothing new under the sun. The object of these diverse illustrations is to impress the meaning I wish to convey, by the phrase of stable forms or groupings, which, however uncertain it may be in outline, is perfectly distinct in substance. Every one of the meanings that have been attached by writers to the vague but convenient word '' type " has for its central idea the existence of a limited number ^ The Royal Coohery Book By Jules GoufF^, Clief de Cuisine of the Paris Jockey Club ; translated by Alphonse Gouffe, Head Pastry Cook to H.M. the Queen. Sampson Low. 1869. Introduction, p. 9. III.] ORGANIC STABILITY. 25 of frequently recurrent forms. The word etymologically compares these forms to the identical medals that may be struck by one or other of a set of dies. The central idea on which the phrase " stable forms " is based is of the same kind, while the phrase further accounts for their origin, vaguely it may be, but still significantly, by showing that though we know little or nothing of details, the result of organic groupings is analogous to much that we notice elsewhere on every side. Siihordwate positions of Stability. — Of course there are different degrees of stability. If the same structural form recurs in successively descending generations, its stability must be great, otherwise it could not have withstood the effects of the admixture of equal doses of alien elements in successive generations. Such a form well deserves to be called typical. A breeder would always be able to establish it. It tends of itself to become a new and stable variety ; therefore all the breeder has to attend to is to give fair play to its tendency, by weeding out from among its offspring such reversions to other forms as may crop up from time to time, and by preserving the breed from rival admixtures until it has become confirmed, and adapted in every minute particular to its surroundings. Personal Forms may be compared to Human Inven- tions, as these also may be divided iuto types, sub-types, and deviations from them. Every important inven- tion is a new type, and of such a definite kind as to admit of clear verbal description, and so of becoming 26 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. the subject of patent rights ; at the same time it need not be so minutely defined as to exclude the possibility of small improvements or of deviations from the main design, any of which may be freely adopted by the in- ventor without losing the protection of his patent. But the range of protection is by no means sharply distinct, as most inventors know to their cost. Some other man, who may or may not be a plagiarist, applies for a sepa- rate patent for himself, on the ground that he has intro- duced modifications of a fundamental character ; in other words, that he has created a fresh type. His application is opposed, and the question whether his plea be valid or not, becomes a subject for legal decision. Whenever a patent is granted subsidiary to another, and lawful to be used only by those who have acquired rights to w^ork the primary invention, then we should rank the new patent as a secondary and not as a primary type. Thus we see that mechanical inventions ofi'er good examples of types, sub-types, and mere deviations. The three kinds of public carriages that characterise the streets of London ; namely, omnibuses, hansoms, and four-wheelers, are specific and excellent illustra- tions of what I wish to express by mechanical types, as distinguished from sub -types. Attempted improve- ments in each of them are yearly seen, but none have as yet superseded the old familiar patterns, which cannot, as it thus far appears, be changed with advantage, taking the circumstances of London as they are. Yet there have been numerous subsidiary and patented contriv- III.] ORGANIC STABILITY. 27 ances, each a distinct step in the improvement of one or other of the three primary types, and there are or may be in each of the three an indefinite number of varieties in details, too unimportant to be subjects of patent rights. The broad classes, of primary or subordinate types, and of mere deviations from them, are separated by no well-defined frontiers. Still the distinction is very ser- viceable, so much so that the whole of the laws of patent and copyright depend upon it, and it forms the only foundation for the title to a vast amount of valuable property. Corresponding forms of classification must be equally appropriate to the organic structure of all living things. Model. — The distinction between primary and sub- ordinate positions of stability will be made clearer by the riG.T help of Fig 1, which is drawn from a model I made. The model has more sides, but Fig. 1 suffices for illustration. It is a polygonal slab that can be made to stand on any one of its edges when set upon a level table, and is 28 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [cbap. intended to illustrate tlie meaning of primary and sub- ordinate stability in organic structures, although the conditions of these must be far more complex than anything we have wits to imagine. The model and the organic structure have the cardinal fact in common, that if either is disturbed without transgressing the range of its stability, it will tend to re-establish itself, but if the range is overpassed it will topple over into a new position ; also that both of them are more likely to topple over towards the position of primary stability, than away from it. The ultimate point to be illustrated is this. Though a long established race habitually breeds true to its kind, subject to small unstable deviations, yet every now and then the offspring of these deviations do not tend to revert, but possess some small stability of their own. They therefore have the character of sub-types, always, however, with a reserved tendency under strained con- ditions, to revert to the earlier type. The model further illustrates the fact that sometimes a sport may occur of such marked peculiarity and stability as to rank as a new type, capable of becoming the origin of a new race with very little assistance on the part of natural selection. Also, that a new type may be reached without any large single stride, but through a fortunate and rapid succession of many small ones. The model is a polygonal slab, the polygon being one that might have been described within an oval, and it is so shaped as to stand on any one of its edges. When the slab rests as in Fig. 1, on the edge A B, corresponding to III.] ORGANIC STABILITY. 29 the shorter diameter of the oval, it stands in its most stable position, and in one from which it is equally diffi- cult to dislodge it by a tilt either forwards or backwards. So long as it is merely tilted it will fall back on being left alone, and its position when merely tilted corre- sponds to a simple deviation. But when it is pushed with sufficient force, it will tumble on to the next edge, B c, into a new position of stability. It will rest there, but less securely than in its first position ; moreover its range of stability will no longer be dis- posed symmetrically. A comparatively slight push from the front will suffice to make it tumble back, a com- paratively heavy push from behind is needed to make it tumble forward. If it be tumbled over into a third position (not shown in the Fig.), the process just described may recur with exaggerated effect, and similarly for many subsequent ones. If, however, the slab is at length brought to rest on the edge c d, most nearly corresponding to its longest diameter, the next onward push, which may be very slight, will suffice to topple it over into an entirely new system of stability ; in other words, a ''sport" comes suddenly into exist- ence. Or the figure might have been drawn with its longest diameter passing into a projecting spur, so that a push of extreme strength would be required to topple it entirely over. If the first position, A B, is taken to represent a type, the other portions will represent sub-types. All the stable positions on the same side of the longer diameter are subordinate to the first position. On whichever of 30 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. of them the polygon may stand, its principal tendency on being seriously disturbed will be to fall back towards the first position ; yet each position is stable within certain limits. Consequently the model illustrates how the following conditions may co- exist : (1) Variability within narrow limits without prejudice to the purity of the breed. (2) Partly stable sub-types. (3) Tendency, when much disturbed, to revert from a sub-type to an earlier form. (4) Occasional sports which may give rise to new types. Stability of Sports. — Experience does not show that those wide varieties which are called ^' sports " are unstable. On the contrary, they are often transmitted to successive generations with curious persistence. Neither is there any reason for expecting otherwise. While we can well understand that a strained modi- fication of a type would not be so stable as one that approximates more nearly to the typical centre, the variety may be so wide that it falls into difi*erent condi- tions of stability, and ceases to be a strained modification of the original type. The hansom cab was originally a marvellous novelty. In the language of breeders it was a sudden and re- markable " sport," yet the suddenness of its appearance has been no bar to its unchanging hold on popular favour. It is not a monstrous anomaly of incongruous parts, and therefore unstable, but quite the contrary. Many other instances of very novel and yet stable inventions could be quoted. One of the earliest III.] ORGANIC STABILITY. 31 electrical batteries was that wliicli is still known as a Grove battery, being the invention of Sir William Grove. Its principle was quite new at the time, and it continues in use without alteration. The persistence in inheritance of trifling characteristics, such as a mole, a white tuft of hair, or multiple fingers, has often been remarked. The reason of it is, I presume, that such characteristics have inconsiderable influence upon the general organic stability ; they are mere excrescences, that may be associated with very different types, and are therefore inheritable without let or hindrance. It seems to me that stability of type, about which we as yet know very little, must be an important factor in the general theory of heredity, when the theory is applied to cases of high breeding. It will be shown later on, at what point a separate allowance requires to be made for it. But in the earlier and principal part of the inquiry, which deals with the inheritance of qualities that are only exceptional in a small degree, a separate allowance does not appear to be required. Infertility of Mixed Types. — It is not difficult to see in a general way why very different types should refuse to. coalesce, and it is scarcely possible to explain the reason why, more clearly than by an illustration. Thus a useful blend between a four-wheeler and a hansom would be impossible ; it would have to run on three wheels and the half-way position for the driver would be upon its roof A blend would be equally impossible 32 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. between an omnibus and a hansom, and it would be difficult between an omnibus and a four-wheeler. Evolution not hy Minute Steps Only. — The theory of Natural Selection might dispense wdth a restriction, for which it is difficult to see either the need or the justification, namely, that the course of evolution always proceeds by steps that are severally minute, and that become effective only through accumulation. That the steps may be small and that they '}nust be small are very different views ; it is only to the latter that I object, and only when the indefinite word *^ small " is used in the sense of " barely discernible," or as small com- pared with such large sports as are known to have been the origins of new races. An apparent ground for the common belief is founded on the fact that whenever search is made for intermediate forms between widely divergent varieties, whether they be of plants or of animals, of weapons or utensils, of customs, religion or language, or of any other product of evolution, a long and orderly series can usually be made out, each member of which differs in an almost imperceptible degree from the adjacent specimens. But it does not at all follow because these intermediate forms have been found to exist, that they are the very stages that w^ere passed through in the course of evolution. Counter evidence exists in abundance, not only of the appearance of con- siderable sports, but of their remarkable stability in hereditary transmission. Many of the specimens of intermediate forms may have been unstable varieties, III.] ORGANIC STABILITY. 33 whose descendants had reverted ; they might be looked upon as tentative and faltering steps taken along parallel courses of evolution, and afterwards retraced. Affiliation from each generation to the next requires to be proved before any apparent line of descent can be accepted as the true one. The history of inventions fully illus- trates this view. It is a most common experience that what an inventor knew to be original, and believed to be new, had been invented independently by others many times before, but had never become established. Even when it has new features, the inventor usually finds, on consulting lists of patents, that other inventions closely border on his own. Yet we know that inventors often proceed by strides, their ideas originating in some sudden happy thought suggested by a chance occurrence, though their crude ideas may have to be laboriously worked out afterwards. If, however, all the varieties of any machine that had ever been invented, were collected and arranged in a Museum in the apparent order of their Evolution, each would differ so little from its neighbour as to suggest the fallacious inference that the successive inventors of that machine had progressed by means of a very large number of hardly discernible steps. The object of this and of the preceding chapter has been first to dwell on the fact of inheritance beinof "particulate," secondly to show how this fact is com- patible with the existence of various types, some of which are subordinate to others, and thirdly to argue 34 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. hi. that Evolution need not proceed by small steps only. I have largely used metaphor and illustratioD to explain the facts, wishing to avoid entanglements with theory as far as possible, inasmuch as no complete theory of inheritance has yet been propounded that meets with general acceptation. CHAPTEK TV, SCHEMES OF DISTEIBUTION AIN'D OF FEEQUENCY. Fraternities and Populations to be treated as Units. — Scliemes of Distribu- tion and their Grades. — The Shape of Schemes is independent of the number of observations. — Data for Eighteen Schemes. — Application of the method of Schemes to inexact Measures. — Schemes of Fre- quency, Fraternities and Populations to he Treated as Units. — The science of heredity is concerned with Fraternities and large Populations rather than with individnals, and mnst treat them as units. A compendious method is therefore requisite by which we may express the dis- tribution of each faculty among the members of any large group, whether it be a Fraternity or an entire Population. The knowledge of an average value is a meagre piece of information. How little is conveyed by the bald statement that the average income of English families is 100^. a year, compared with what we should learn if w^e were told how English incomes were distributed ; what proportion of our countrymen had just and only just enough means to ward off starvation, and what were the D 2 36 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [ohap. proportions of those who had incomes in each and every other degree, up to the huge annual receipts of a few great speculators, manufacturers, and landed proprietors. So in respect to the distribution of any human quality or faculty, a knowledge of mere averages tells but little ; we want to learn how the quality is distributed among the various members of the Fraternity or of the Popula- tion, and to express what we know in so compact a form that it can be easily grasped and dealt with. A parade of great accuracy is foolish, because precision is unattainable in biological and social statistics ; their results being never strictly constant. Over-minuteness is mischievous, because it overwhelms the mind with more details than can be compressed into a single view. We require no more than a fairly just and comprehensive method of expressing the way in which each measurable quality is distributed among the members of any group, whether the group consists of brothers or of members of any particular social, local, or other body of persons, or whether it is co- extensive with an entire nation or race. A knowledge of the distribution of any quality en- ables us to ascertain the Eank that each man holds among his fellows, in respect to that quality. This is a valuable piece of knowledge in this struggling and competitive world, where success is to the foremost, and failure to the hindmost, irrespective of absolute efficiency. A blurred vision would be above all price to an in- dividual man in a nation of blind men, though it would hardly enable him to earn his bread elsewhere. When IV.] SCHEMES OF DISTRIBUTION AND OF FREQUENCY. 37 tlie distribution of any faculty has been ascertained, we can tell from the measurement, say of our child, how he ranks among other children in respect to that faculty, whether it be a physical gift, or one of health, or of intellect, or of morals. As the years go by, we may learn by the same means whether he is making his way towards the front, whether he just holds his place, or whether he is falling back towards the rear. Similarly as regards the position of our class, or of our nation, among other classes and other nations. Schemes of Distribution and their Grades. — I shall best explain my graphical method of expressing Dis- tribution, which I like the more, the more I use it, and which I have latterly much developed, by showing how to determine the Grrade of an individual among his fellows in respect to any particular faculty. Suppose that we have already put on record the measures of many men in respect to Strength, exerted as by an archer in pulling his bow, and tested by one of Salter's well-known dial instruments with a movable index. Some men will have been found strong and others weak ; how can we picture in a compendious diagram, or how can we define by figures, the distribution of this faculty of Strength throughout the group ? How shall we determine and specify the Grade that any particular person would occupy in the group ? The first step is to marshal our measures in the orderly way familiar to statisticians, which is shown in Table I. I usually work to about twice its degree of minuteness, but enough 38 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. has been entered in the Table for the purpose of illnstration, while its small size makes it all the more intellioible. The fourth column of the Table headed " Percentages " of " Sums from the beginning," is pictorially translated into Fig. 2, and the third column headed " Percentages " of " No. of cases observed," into Fis^. 3. The scale of FIG. 2. Us 100 80 -GO 40 •''-20 MiO FIG. 3. 91 W41 kjil||l|W|i||in |i'i:nj!iIiiimiI 4D — r- 20 Ths. 100 so 60 -40 '20 i-0 lbs. is given at the side of both Figs. : and the com- partments a to g, that are shaded with hrohen lines, have the same meaning in both, but they are differently disposed in the two Figs. We will now consider Fig. 2 only, which is the one that principally concerns us. The percentages in the last column of Table I. have been marked off on the bottom line of Fig. 2, where they are called (centesimal) Grades. The number of lbs. found in the first column of the Table determines IV.] SCHEMES OF DISTRIBUTION AND OF FREQUENCY. 39 the height of the vertical lines to be erected at the corresponding Grades when we are engaged in con- structing the Figure. Let us bemn with the third line in the Table for illustration : it tells us that 37 per cent, of the group had Strengths less than 70 lbs. Therefore, when drawing the figure, a perpendicular must be raised at the 37th grade to a height corresponding to that of 70 lbs. on the side scale. The fourth line in the Table tells us that 70 per cent, of the group had Strengths less than 80 lbs. ; therefore a perpendicular must be raised at the 70th Grade to a height corresponding to 80 lbs. We proceed in the same way with respect to the remaining figures, then we join the tops of these perpendiculars by straight lines. As these observations of Strength have been sorted into only 7 groups, the trace formed by the lines that connect the tops of the few perpendiculars difi'ers sensibly from a flowing curve, but when working with double minuteness, as mentioned above, the connecting lines difi"er little to the eye from the dotted curve. The dotted curve may then be accepted as that which would result if a separate perpendicular had been drawn for every observation, and if permission had been given to slightly smooth their irregularities. I call the figure that is bounded by such a curve as this, a Scheme of Distribution ; the perpendiculars that formed the scaf- folding by which it was constructed having been first rubbed out. (See Fig 4, next page.) A Scheme enables us in a moment to find the Grade 40 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. of Eank (on a scale reckoned from 0° to 100°) of any person in the gronp to which he belongs. The measured strength of the person is to be looked for in the side scale of the Scheme ; a horizontal line is thence drawn until it meets the curve ; from the point of meeting a perpendicular is dropped upon the scale of Grades at the base ; then the G-rade on which it falls is FIG .4. 1 y^ - / • 100 FIG. 5. 50 £0° 100 IDO" 100* the one required. For example : let us suppose the Strength of Pull of a man to have been 74 lbs., and that we wish to determine his Eank in Strength among the large group of men who were measured at the Health Exhibition in 1884. We find by Fig. 4 that his centesimal Grade is 50°; in other words, that 50 per cent, of the group will be weaker than he is, and 50 per cent, will be stronger. His IV.] SCHEMES OF DISTRIBUTION AND OF FREQUENCY. 41 position will be exactly Middlemost, after tlie Strengths of all the men in the gronp have been marshalled in the order of their magnitudes. In other words, he is of mediocre strength. The accepted term to express the value that occupies the Middlemost position is " Median," which may be used either as an adjective or as a substantive, but it will be usually replaced in this book by the abbreviated form M. I also use the word " Mid " in a few combinations, such as " Mid-Fraternity," to express the same thing. The Median, M, has three properties. The first follows immediately from its con- struction, namely, that the chance is an equal one, of any previously unknown measure in the group exceeding or falling short of M. The second is, that the most probable value of any previously unknown measure in the group is M. Thus if N be any one of the measures, and u be the value of the unit in which the measure is recorded, such as an inch, tenth of an inch, &c., then the number of measures that fall between (N - ^u) and (N -\r^u), is greatest when N=: IVI. Mediocrity is always the commonest condition, for reasons that will become apparent later on. The third property is that whenever the curve of the Scheme is symmetrically disposed on either side of M, except that one half of it is turned upwards, and the other half downwards, then M is identical with the ordinary Arithmetic Mean or Average. This is closely the condition of all the curves I have to discuss. The reader may look on the Median and on the Mean as being practically the same things, throughout this book. 42 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. It must be understood that M, like the Mean or the Average, is almost always an interpolated value, corre- sponding to no real measure. If the observations were infinitely numerous its position would not differ more than infinitesimally from that of some one of them ; even in a series of one or two hundred in number, the difference is insignificant. Now let us make our Scheme answer another question. Suppose we want to know the percentage of men in the group of which we have been speaking, whose Strength lies between any two specified limits, as between 74 lbs. and 64 lbs. We draw horizontal lines (Fig. 4) from points on the side scale corresponding to either limit, and drop perpendiculars upon the base, from the points where those lines meet the curve. Then the number of Grades in the intercept is the answer. The Fig. shows that the number in the present case is 30 ; therefore 30 per cent, of the group. have Strengths of Pull ranging between 74 and 64 lbs. We learn how to transmute female measures of any characteristic into male ones, by comparing their respec- tive schemes, and devising a formula that will change the one into the other. In the case of Stature, the simple multiple of 1*08 was found to do this with sufficient precision. If we wish to compare the average Strengths of two different groups of persons, say one consisting of men and the other of women, we have simply to comjDare the values at the 50th Grades in the two schemes. For even if the Medians difi'cr considerably from the Means, IV.] SCHEMES OF DISTRIBUTION AND OF FREQUENCY. 43 both the ratios and the differences between either pair of values would be sensibly the same. A different way of comparing two Schemes is some- times useful. It is to draw them in opposed directions, as in Fig. 5, p. 40. Their curves will then cut each other at some point, whose Grade when referred to either of the two Schemes (whichever of them may be preferred), determines the point at which the same values are to be found. In Fig. 5, the Grade in the one Scheme is 20° ; therefore in the other Scheme it is 100°- 20°, or 80°. In respect to the Strength of Pull of men and women, it appears that the woman who occupies the Grade of 96° in her Scheme, has the same strength as the man who occupies the Grade of 4° in his Scheme. I should add that this great inequality in Strength between the sexes, is confirmed by other measure- ments made at the same time in respect to the Strength of their Squeeze, as tested by another of Salter's instruments. Then the woman in the 93rd and the man in the 7th Grade of their resective Schemes, proved to be of equal strength. In my paper ^ on the results obtained at the laboratory, I remarked : " Yery powerful women exist, but happily perhaps for the repose of the other sex such gifted women are rare. Out of 1,657 adult women of all ages measured at the laboratory, the strongest could only exert a squeeze of 86 lbs., or about that of a medium man." 1 Journ. Anthropol. Inst. 1885. Mem. : There is a blunder in the para- graph, p. 23, headed "Height Sitting and Standing." The paragraph should be struck out. 44 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. The Shape of Schemes is Independent of the Number of Observations. — When Schemes are drawn from dif- ferent samples of the same large group of measurements, though the number in the several samples may differ greatly, we can always so adjust the horizontal scales that the breadth of the several Schemes shall be uniform. Then the shaj)es of the Schemes drawn from different samples will be little affected by the number of observa- tions used in each, supposing of course that the numbers are never too small for ordinary statistical purposes. The only recognisable differences between the Schemes will be, that, if the number of observations in the sample is very large, the upper margin of the Scheme will fall into a more regular curve, especially towards either of its limits. Some irregularity will be found in the above curve of the Strength of Pull ; but if the observations had been ten times more numerous, it is probable, judging from much experience of such curves, that the irregularity would have been less consjoicuous, and perhaps would have disappeared altogether. However numerous the observations may be, the curve will always be uncertain and incomplete at its extreme ends, because the next value may happen to be greater or less than any one of those that preceded it. Again, the position of the first and the last observation, supposing each observation to have been laid down sepa- rately, can never coincide with the adjacent limit. The more numerous the observations, and therefore the closer the perpendiculars by which they are represented, the nearer will the two extreme perpendiculars approach the IV] SCHEiMES OF DISTRIBUTION AND OF FREQUENCY. 45 limits, but they will never actually touch them. A chess board has eight squares in a row, and eight pieces may be arranged in order on any one row, each piece occupying the centre of a square. Let the divisions in the row be graduated, calling the boundary to the extreme left, 0°. Then the successive divisions between the squares will be 1°, 2°, 3°, up to 7°, and the boundary to the extreme right will be 8°. It is clear that the position of the first piece lies half-way between the grades (in a scale of eight grades) of 0° and 1° ; therefore the grade occupied by the first piece would be counted on that scale as 0*5°; also the grade of the last piece as 7 '5°. Or again, if we had 800 pieces, and the same number of class-places, the grade of the first piece, in a scale of 800 grades, would exceed the grade 0°, by an amount equal to the width of one half-place on that scale, while the last of them would fall short of the 800th grade by an equal amount. This half-place has to be attended to and allowed for when schemes are con- structed from comparatively few observations, and always when values that are very near to either of the centesimal grades 0° or 100° are under observation ; but between the centesimal grades of 5° and 95° the influence of a half class-place upon the value of the corresponding observation is insignificant, and may be disregarded. It will not henceforth be necessary to repeat the word centesimal. It will be always implied when nothing is said to the contrary, and nothing henceforth will be said to the contrary. The word will be used for the last time in the next paragraph. 46 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. Data for Eighteen Schemes. — Sufficient data for re- constructing any Scheme, witli niucli correctness, may be printed in a single line of a Table, and according to a uniform plan tbat is suitable for any kind of values. The measures to be recorded are those at a few definite G-rades, beginning say at 5°, ending at 95°, and including every intermediate tenth Grade from 10° to 90°. It is convenient to add those at the Grades 25° and 75°, if space permits. The former values are given for eighteen different Schemes, in Table 2. In the memoir from which that table is reprinted, the values at what I now call (centesimal) Grades, were termed Percentiles. Thus the values at the Grades 5° and 10° would be respectively the 5th and the 10th percentile. It still seems to me that the word percentile is a useful and expressive abbreviation, but it will not be necessary to employ it in the present book. It is of course unadvisable to use more technical words than is absolutely necessary, and it will be possible to get on without it, by the help of the new and more important word " Grade." A series of Schemes that express the distribution of various faculties, is valuable in an anthropometric labora- tory, for they enable every person who is measured to find his Rank or Grade in each of them. Diagrams may also be constructed by drawing parallel lines, each divided into 100 Grades, and entering each round number of inches, lbs., &c., at their proper places. A diagram of this kind is very convenient for reference, but it does not admit of being printed ; it must be drawn or lithographed. I have constructed one of these IV.] SCHEMES OF DISTRIBUTION AND OF FREQUENCY. 47 from tlie 18 Schemes, and find it is easily understood and mucli used at my laboratory. Application of Schemes to Inexact Measures. — Schemes of Distribution may be constructed from observations that are barely exact enough to deserve to be called measures. I will illustrate the method of doing so by marshalling the data contained in a singularly interesting little memoir written by Sir James Paget, into the form of such a Scheme. The memoir is published in vol. v. of St. Bartholomew's Hospital Eeports, and is entitled " What Becomes of Medical Students." He traced with great painstaking the career of no less than 1,000 pupils who had attended his classes at that Hospital during various periods and up to a date 15 years previous to that at which his memoir was written. He thus did for St. Bartholomew's Hospital what has never yet been done, so far as I am aware, for any University or Public School, whose historians count the successes and are silent as to the failures, giving to inquirers no adequate data for ascertaining the real value of those institutions in English Education. Sir J. Paget divides the successes of his pupils in their profession into fiYQ grades, all of which he carefully defines ; they are distinguished ; considerable ; moderate ; very limited success ; and failures. Several of the students had left the profes- sion either before or after taking their degrees, usually owing to their unfitness to succeed, so after analysing the accounts of them given in the memoir, I drafted 48 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. several into the list of failures and distributed the rest, with the result that the number of cases in the successive classes, amounting now to the full total of 1,000, became 28, 80, 616, 151, and 125. This differs, I should say, a Kttle from the inferences of the author, but the matter is here of small importance, so I need not go further into details. If a Scheme is drawn from these figures, in the way described in page 39, it will be found to have the characteristic shape of our familiar curve of Distribution. If we wished to convey the utmost information that this Scheme is capable of giving, we might record in much detail the career of two or three of the men who are clustered about each of a few selected Grades, such as those that are used in Table 11. , or fewer of them. I adopted this method when estimating the variability of the Visualising Power (Inquiries into Human Faculty). My data were very lax, but this method of treatment got all the good out of them that they possessed. In the present case, it appears that towards the foremost of the successful men within fifteen years of taking their degrees, stood the three Professors of Anatomy at Oxford, Cambridge, and Edinburgh ; that towards the bottom of the failures, lay two men who committed suicide under circumstances of great disgrace, and lowest of all Palmer, the Rugeley murderer, who was hanged. We are able to compare any two such Schemes as the above, with numerical precision. The want of exactness in the data from which they are drawn, will of course cling to the result, but no new error will l^e introduced IV.] SCHEMES OF DISTRIBUTION AND OF FREQUENCY. 49 by the process of comparison. Suppose the second Scheme to refer to the successes of students from another hospital, we should draw the two Schemes in opposed directions, just as was done in the Strength of Pull of Males and Females, Fig. 5, and determine the Grrade in either of the Schemes at which success was equal. Schemes of Frequency. — The method of arranging observations in an orderly manner that is generally employed by statisticians, is shown in Fig. 3, page 38, which expresses the same facts as Fig. 2 under a different aspect, and so gives rise to the well-known Curve of "Frequency of Error," though in Fig. 3 the curve is turned at right angles to the position in which it is usually drawn. It is so placed in order to show more clearly its relation to the Curve of Distribution. The Curve of Frequency is far less convenient than that of Distribution, for the purposes just described and for most of those to be hereafter spoken of. But the Curve of Frequency has other uses, of which advantage will be taken later on, and to which it is unnecessary now to refer. A Scheme as explained thus far, is nothing more than a compendium of a mass of observations which, on being marshalled in an orderly manner, fall into a diagram whose contour is so regular, simple, and bold, as to admit of being described by a few numerals (Table 2), from which it can at any time be drawn afresh. The regular distribution of the several faculties among a large population is little disturbed by the fact that its E 50 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. iv. members are varieties of different types and sub-types. So the distribution of a heavy mass of foliage gives little indication of its growth from separate twigs, of separate branches, of separate trees. The application of theory to Schemes, their approxi- mate description by only two values, and the properties of their bounding Curves, will be described in the next chapter. CHAPTER y. NOEMAL VAEIABILITY. Schemes of Deviations. — Normal Curve of Distribution. — Comparison of the observed with the Normal Curve. — Tlie value of a single Devia- tion at a known Grade determines a Normal Scheme of Deviations. — Two Measures at two known Grades determine a Normal Scheme of Measures. — The Charms of Statistics. — Mechanical illustration of the Cause of the Curve of Frequency. — Order in apparent Chaos. — Problems in the Law of Error. Schemes of Deviations. — We have now seen how easy it is to represent the distribution of any qnality among a multitude of men, either by a simple diagram or by a line containing a few figures. In this chapter it will be shown that a considerably briefer description is approximately sufficient. Every measure in a Scheme is equal to its Middlemost, or Median value, or ^,plus or minus a certain Devia- tion from M. The Deviation, or "Error" as it is technically called, is plus for all grades above 50°, zero for 50°, and minus for all grades below 50°. Thus if (±D) be the deviation from IVI in any particular case, every measure in a Scheme may be expressed in the E 2 52 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. form of IV1 + (±D). If IVI = 0, or if it is subtracted from every measure, the residues wliicli are tlie different values of ( ± D) will form a Scheme by themselves. Schemes may therefore be made of Deviations as well as of Measures, and one of the former is seen in the upper part of Fig. 6, page 40. It is merely the upper portion of the corresponding Scheme of Measures, in which the axis of the curve plays the part of the base. A strong family likeness runs between the 1 8 different Schemes of Deviations that may be respectively derived from the data in the 18 lines of Table 2. If the slope of the curve in one Scheme is steeper than that of another, we need only to fore-shorten the steeper Scheme, by inclining it away from the line of sight, in order to reduce its apparent steepness and to make it look almost identical with the other. Or, better still, we may select appropriate vertical scales that will enable all the Schemes to be drawn afresh with a uniform slope, and be made strictly comparable. Suppose that we have only two Schemes, A. and b., that we wish to compare. Let L.i, L.2 be the lengths of the perpendiculars at two specified grades in Scheme A., and K.i K.2 the lengths of those at the same grades in Scheme b. ; then if every one of the data from which Scheme b. was drawn be multiplied by ^^ ^'^' , a series of transmuted data will be obtained for drawing a new Scheme b'., on such a vertical scale that its general slope between the selected grades shall be the same as in Scheme A. For practical convenience the v.] NORMAL VARIABILITY. 53 selected Grades will be always those of 25° and 75°. They stand at the first and third quarterly divisions of the base, and are therefore easily found by a pair of compasses. They are also well placed to afford a fair criterion of the general slope of the Curve. If we call the perpendicular at 25°, Q.i ; and that at 75°, Q.2, then the unit by which every Scheme will be defined is its value of -^(0.2- Q-i), and will be called its Q. As the M measures the Average Height of the curved boundary of a Scheme, so the Q measures its general slope. When we wish to transform many difi'er- ent Schemes, numbered I., II., III., &c., whose respective values of Q are qi, q^, q^, Sec, to others whose values of Q are in each case equal to qo, then all the data from which Scheme I. was drawn, must be multiplied by ^ ; those from which Scheme II. was drawn, by ^, and so on, and new Schemes have to be constructed from these trans- muted values. Our Q has the further merit of being practically the same as the value which mathematicians call the " Probable Error," of which we shall speak further on. Want of space in Table 2 prevented the insertion of the measures at the Grades 25° and 75°, but those at 20° and 30° are given on the one hand, and those at 70** and 80° on the other, whose respective averages diff'er but little from the values at 25° and 75°. I therefore will use those four measures to obtain a value for our unit, which we will call Q^, to distinguish it from Q. 54 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. These are not identical in value, because the outline of the Scheme is a curved and not a straight line, but the difference between them is small, and is approximately the same in all Schemes. It will shortly be seen that Q'= 1*015 xQ approximately; therefore a series of De- viations measured in terms of the large unit Q' are numerically smaller than if they had been measured in terms of the small unit (for the same reason that the numerals in 2, 3, kQ.^feet are smaller than those in the corresponding values of 24, 36, &c., inches), and they must be multiplied by 1.015 when it is desired to change them into a series having the smaller value of Q for their unit. All the 18 Schemes of Deviation that can be derived from Table 2 have been treated on these principles, and the results are given in Table 3. Their general accord- ance with one another, and still more with the mean of all of them, is obvious. Normal Curve of Distribution. — The values in the bottom line of Table 3, which is headed " Normal Values when Q = 1," and which correspond with minute pre- cision to those in the line immediately above them, are not derived from observations at all, but from the well- known Tables of the " Probability Integral " in a way that mathematicians will easily understand by comparing the Tables 4 to 8 inclusive. I need hardly remind the reader that the Law of Error upon which these Normal Values are based, was excogitated for the use of astro- nomers and others who are concerned with extreme v.] NORMAL VARIABILITY. 55 accuracy of measurement, and without tlie slightest idea until the time of Quetelet that they might be applicable to human measures. But Errors, Differences, Deviations, Divergencies, Dispersions, and individual Variations, all spring from the same kind of causes. Objects that bear the same name, or can be described by the same phrase, are thereby acknowledged to have common points of resemblance, and to rank as members of the same species, class, or whatever else we may please to call the group. On the other hand, every object has Differences peculiar to itself, by which it is distinguished from others. This general statement is applicable to thousands of instances. The Law of Error finds a footing wherever the individual peculiarities are wholly due to the com- bined influence of a multitude of " accidents," in the sense in which that word has already been defined. All persons conversant with statistics are aware that this supposition brings Variability within the grasp of the laws of Chance, with the result that the relative frequency of Deviations of different amounts admits of being calculated, when those amounts are measured in terms of any self-contained unit of varia- bility, such as our Q. The Tables 4 to 8 give the results of these purely mathematical calculations, and the Curves based upon them may with propriety be distinguished as '' Normal." Tables 7 and 8 are based upon the familiar Table of the Probability Integral, given in Table 5, via that in Table 6, in which the unit of variability is taken to be the " Probable Error " or our Q, and not the "Modulus." Then I turn Table 6 56 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. inside out, as it were, deriving the " arguments " for Tables 7 and 8 from tlie entries in the body of Table 6, and making other easily intelligible alterations. Comparison of the Observed ivith the Normal Curve. — I confess to having been amazed at the extraordinary coincidence between the two bottom lines of Table 3, considering the great variety of faculties contained in the 1 8 Schemes ; namely, three kinds of linear measure- ment, besides one of weight, one of capacity, two of strength, one of vision, and one of swiftness. It is obvious that weight cannot really vary at the same rate as height, even allowing for the fact that tall men are often lanky, but the theoretical impossibility is of the less practical importance, as the variations in weight are small compared to the weight itself. Thus we see from the value of Q in the first column of Table 3, that half of the persons deviated from their M by no more than 10 or 11 lbs., which is about one-twelfth part of the value of IVI. Although the several series in Table 3 run fairly well together, I should not have dared to hope that their irregularities would have balanced one another so beautifully as they have done. It has been objected to some of my former work, especially in Hereditary Genius, that I pushed the applications of the Law of Frequency of Error somewhat too far. I may have done so, rather by incautious phrases than in reality ; but I am sure that, with the evidence now before us, the applicability of that law is more than justified within the reasonable limits asked for in the present book. I v.] NORMAL VARIABILITY. 57 am satisfied to claim that the Normal Curve is a fair average representation of the Observed Curves during nine-tenths of their course ; that is, for so much of them as lies between the grades of 5° and 95°. In particular, the agreement of the Curve of Stature with the Normal Curve is very fair, and forms a mainstay of my inquiry into the laws of Natural Inheritance. It has already been said that mathematicians laboured at the law of Error for one set of purposes, and we are entering into the fruits of their labours for another. Hence there is no ground for surprise that their Nomen- clature is often cumbrous and out of place, when applied to problems in heredity. This is especially the case with regard to their term of " Probable Error," by which they mean the value that one half of the Errors exceed and the other half fall short of This is practically the same as our Q.-^ It is strictly the same whenever the two halves of the Scheme of Deviations to which it applies are symmetrically disposed about their common axis. The term Probable Error, in its plain English inter- pretation of the most Probable Error, is quite mis- leading, for it is not that. The most Probable Error (as Dr. Venn has pointed out, in his Logic of Chance) 1 The following little Table may be of service : — Values of the different Constants when the Proh. Error is tahen as unity, and their corresponding Grades. Prob. Error I'OOO Modulus 2-097 Mean Error 1-183 Error of Mean Squares 1-483 corresponding Grades 25°-0, 75° -0 r-9, 92°-l „ „ 21°-2, 78°-8 16°-0, 84°-0 58 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. is zero. This results from what was said a few pages back about the most probable measure in a Scheme being its IVI. In a Scheme of Errors the M is equal to 0, therefore the most Probable Error in such a Scheme is 0 also. It is astonishing that mathematicians, who are the most precise and perspicacious of men, have not long since revolted against this cumbrous, slip-shod, and misleading phrase. They really mean what I should call the Mid-Error, but their phrase is too firmly established for me to uproot it. I shall however always write the word Probable when used in this sense, in the form of " Prob. " ; thus " Prob. Error," as a continual protest against its illegitimate use, and as some slight safeguard against its misinterpretation. Moreover the term Probable Error is absurd when applied to the subjects now in hand, such as Stature, Eye-colour, Artistic Faculty, or Disease. I shall therefore usually speak of Prob. Deviation. Though the value of our Q is the same as that of the Prob. Deviation, Q is not a convertible term with Prob. Deviation. We shall often have to speak of the one without immediate reference to the other, just as we speak of the diameter of the circle without reference to any of its properties, such as, if lines are drawn from its ends to any point in the circumference, they will meet at a right angle. The Q of a Scheme is as de- finite a phrase as the Diameter of a Circle, but we cannot replace Q in that phrase by the words Prob. Deviation, and speak of the Prob. Deviation of a Scheme, without doing some violence to lanoaiao^e. We v.] NORMAL VARIABILITY. 59 should have to express ourselves from another point of view, and at much greater length, and say "the Prob. Deviation of any, as yet unknown measure in the Scheme, from the Mean of all the measures from which the Scheme was constructed." The primary idea of Q has no reference to the existence of a Mean value from which Deviations take place. It is half the difference between the measures found at the 25th and 75th Centesimal Grades. In this definition there is not the slightest allusion, direct or indirect, to the measure at the 50th Grade, which is the value of M. It is perfectly true that the measure at Grade 25° is IVI — Q, and that at Grade 75° is IVI + Q, but all this is superimposed upon the primary conception. Q stands essentially on its own basis, and has nothing to do with IVI. It will often happen that we shall have to deal with Prob : Deviations, but that is no reason why we should not use Q whenever it suits our purposes better, especially as statistical statements tend to be so cum- brous that every abbreviation is welcome. The stage to which we have now arrived is this. It has been shown that the distribution of very different human qualities and faculties is approximately Normal, and it is inferred that with reasonable precautions we may treat them as if they were wholly so, in order to obtain approximate results. We shall thus deal with an entire Scheme of Deviations in terms of its Q, and with an entire Scheme of Measures in terms of its IVI and Q, just as we deal with an entire Circle in terms of its 60 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. radius, or with an entire Ellipse in terms of its major and minor axes. We can also apply tlie various beau- tiful properties of the Law of Frequency of Error to the observed values of Q. In doing so, we act like woodsmen who roughly calculate the cubic contents of the trunk of a tree, by measuring its length, and its girth at either end, and submitting their measures to formulae that have been deduced from the properties of ideally perfect straight lines and circles. Their results prove serviceable, although the trunk is only rudely straight and circular. I trust that my results will be yet closer approximations to the truth than those usually arrived at by the woodsmen. The value of a single Deviation at a known Grade determines a Normal Scheme of Deviations, — When Normal Curves of Distribution are drawn within the same limits, they differ from each other only in their general slope ; and the slope is determined if the value of the Deviation is given at any one specified G-rade. It must be borne in mind that the width of the limits between which the Scheme is drawn, has no influence on the values of the Deviations at the various Grades, because the latter are proportionate parts of the base. As the limits vary in width, so do the intervals between the Grades. When measuring the Deviation at a speci- fied Grade for the purpose of determining the whole Curve, it is of course convenient to adhere to the same Grade in all cases. It will be recollected that when dealing with the observed curves a few pages back, I v.] NORMAL VARIABILITY. 61 used not one Grade but two Grades for the purpose, namely 25° and 75° ; but in tbe Normal Curve, the plus and minus Deviations are equal in amount at all pairs of symmetrical distances on either side of grade 50° ; therefore the Deviation at either of the Grades 25° or 75° is equal to Q, and suffices to define the entire Curve. The reason why a certain value Q' was stated a few pages back to be equal to 1'015 Q, is that the Normal Deviations at 20° and at 30°, (whose average we called Q') are found in Table 8, to be 1*25 and 078; and similarly those at 70° and 60°. The average of 1*25 and 0"78 is 1'015, whereas the Deviation at 25° or at 75° is 1-000. Tivo Measures at known Grades determine a Normal Scheme of Measures. — If we know the value of IVI as well as that of Q we know the entire Scheme. IVI ex- presses the mean value of all the objects contained in the group, and Q defines their variability. But if we know the Measures at any two specified Grades, we can deduce IVI and Q from them, and so determine the entire Scheme. The method of doing this is explained in the foot-note.^ 1 The following is a fuller description of the propositions in this and in the preceding paragraph : — (1) In any Normal Scheme, and therefore approximately in an observed one, if the value of the Deviation is given at any one specified Grade the whole Curve is determined. Let D he the given Deviation, and d the tabular Deviation at the same Grade, as found in Table 8 ; then multiply every entry in Table 8 ^J—r. As the tabular value of |J is 1, it will become changed into _. ^ d 62 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. The Charms of Statistics. — It is difficult to under- stand why statisticians commonly limit their inquiries to Averages, and do not revel in more comprehensive views. Their souls seem as dull to the charm of variety as that of the native of one of our flat English counties, whose retrospect of Switzerland was that, if its moun- tains could be thrown into its lakes, two nuisances would be got rid of at once. An Average is but a solitary fact, whereas if a single other fact be added to it,, an entire Normal Scheme, which nearly corresponds to the observed one, starts potentially into existence. Some people hate the very name of statistics, but I find them full of beauty and interest. Whenever they are not brutalised, but delicately handled by the higher methods, and are warily interpreted, their power of dealing with complicated phenomena is extraordinary. They are the only tools by which an opening can be cut (2) If the Measures at any two specified Grades are given, the whole Scheme of Measures is thereby determined. Let ^, 5 be the two given Measures of which A is the larger, and let a, h be the values of the tabular Deviations for the same Grades, as found in Table 8, not omitting their signs of jplus or minus as the case may be. A —B Then the (J of the Scheme = ± j . (The sign of |J is not to be re- garded ; it is merely a magnitude.) \^ = A - a^; ot\^ = B - h[^. Example : A^ situated at Grade 55°, = 14*38 B, situated at Grade 5", = 9*12 The corresponding tabular Deviations are : — a = -j-0'19; h — — 2 •44. Therefore R = '^'^^ " ^^'^ =^^= 2-0 ^ 0-19 H- 2-44 2-63 M = 14.38 - 0-19 X 2 = 14-0 or = 9-12 + 2-44 X 2 = 14*0 v.] NORMAL VARIABILITY. 63 through the formidable thicket of difficulties that bars the path of those who pursue the Science of man. Mechanical Illustration of the Cause of the Curve of Frequency. — The Curve of Frequency, and that of Dis- tribution, are convertible : therefore if the genesis of either of them can be made clear, that of the other becomes also intelligible. I shall now illustrate the origin of the Curve of Frequency, by means of an apparatus shown in Fig. 7, that mimics in a very pretty way the conditions FIG.T. / ^ V \ • • / / 1 • N 15 FIG .8. A B FIG .9. A B r 1 V > ( c / / ft • / f on which Deviation depends. It is a frame glazed in front, leaving a depth of about a quarter of an inch be- hind the glass. Strips are placed in the upper part to act as a funnel. Below the outlet of the funnel stand a 64 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. succession of rows of pins stuck squarely into tlie back- board, and below these again are a series of vertical compartments. A charge of small sliot is inclosed. When the frame is held topsy-turvy, all the shot runs to the upper end ; then, when it is turned back into its working position, the desired action commences. Lateral strips, shown in the diagram, have the effect of directing all the shot that had collected at the upper end of the frame to run into the wide mouth of the funnel. The shot passes through the funnel and issuing from its narrow end, scampers deviously down through the pins in a curious and interesting way ; each of them darting a step to the right or left, as the case may be, every time it strikes a pin. The pins are disposed in a quincunx fashion, so that every descending shot strikes against a pin in each successive row. The cascade issuing from the funnel broadens as it descends, and, at length, every shot finds itself caught in a compartment immediately after freeing itself from the last row of pins. The outline of the columns of shot that accumulate in the successive compartments approximates to the Curve of Frequency (Fig. 3, p. 38), and is closely of the same shape however often the experiment is re- peated. The outline of the columns would become more nearly identical with the Normal Curve of Frequency, if the rows of pins were much more numerous, the shot smaller, and the compartments narrower ; also if a larger quantity of shot w^as used. The principle on which the action of the apparatus depends is, that a number of small and independent v.] NORMAL VARIABILITY. 65 accidents befall each sliot in its career. In rare cases, a long run of luck continues to favour the course of a particular shot towards either outside place, but in the large majority of instances the number of accidents that cause De^dation to the right, balance in a greater or less degree those that cause Deviation to the left. Therefore most of the shot finds its way into the com- partments that are situated near to a perpendicular line drawn from the outlet of the funnel, and the Frequency with which shots stray to different distances to the right or left of that line diminishes in a much faster ratio than those distances increase. This illustrates and explains the reason why mediocrity is so common. If a larger quantity of shot is put inside the apparatus, the resulting curve will be more humped, but one half of the shot win stiU faU within the same distance as before, reckoning to the right and left of the perpen- dicular line that passes through the mouth of the funnel. This distance, which does not vary with the quantity of the shot, is the " Prob : Error," or " Prob : Deviation," of any single shot, and has the same value as our Q. But a Scheme of Frequency is unsuitable for finding the values of either IVI or Q. To do so, we must divide its strangely shaped area into four equal parts by vertical lines, which is hardly to be efi'ected except by a tedious process of " Trial and Error." On the other hand iVI and Q can be derived from Schemes of Distribution with no more trouble than is needed to divide a line into four equal parts. F 66 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. Order in Apparent Chaos. — I know of scarcely any- thing so apt to impress tlie imagination as the wonderful form of cosmic order expressed by the " Law of Fre- c|iiency of Error." The law would have been personified by the Greeks and deified, if they had known of it. It reigns v/ith serenity and in complete self-effacement amidst the wildest confusion. The huger the mob, and the greater the apparent anarchy, the more perfect is its sway. It is the supreme law of Unreason. Whenever a large sample of chaotic elements are taken in hand and marshalled in the order of their magnitude, an un- suspected and most beautiful form of regularity proves to have been latent all along. The tops of the mar- shalled row form a flowing curve of invariable pro- portions ; and each element, as it is sorted into place, finds, as it were, a pre-ordained niche, accurately adapted to fit it. If the measurement at any two specified Grades- in the row are known, those that will be found at every other Grade, except towards the extreme ends, can be predicted in the way already explained, and with much precision. Prohlems in the Lain of Brror. — All the properties of the Law of Frequency of Error can he expressed in terms of Q, or of the Prob: Error, just as those of a circle can be expressed in terms of its radius. The visible Schemes are not, however, to be removed too soon from our imagination. It is always well to retain a clear geometric view of the facts when we are dealing with statistical problems, which abound with dangerous v.] NORMAL VARIABILITY. G7 pitfalls, easily overlooked by the unwary, while they are cantering gaily along upon their arithmetic. The Laws of Error are beautiful in themselves and exceedingly fascinating to inquirers, owing to the thoroughness and simplicity with which they deal with masses of materials that appear at first sight to be entanglements on the largest scale, and of a hopelessly confused description. I will mention five of the laws. (1) The following is a mechanical illustration of the first of them. In the apparatus already described, let q stand for the Prob: Error of any one of the shots that are dispersed among the compartments BB at its base. Now cut the apparatus in two parts, horizontally through the rows of pins. Separate the parts and interpose a row of vertical compartments A A, as in Fig. 8, p. 63, where the bottom compartments, BB, corresponding to those shown in Fig. 7, are reduced to half their depth, in order to bring the whole figure within the same sized outline as before. The compartments BB are still deep enough for their purpose. It is clear that the inter- polation of the AA compartments can have no ultimate efi'ect on the final dispersion of the shot into those at BB. Now close the bottoms of all the AA compart- ments ; then the shot that falls from the funnel will be retained in them, and will be comparatively little dis- persed. Let the Prob: Error of a shot in the AA com- partments be called a. Next, open the bottom of any one of the AA compartments ; then the shot it contains will cascade downwards and disperse themselves among the BB compartments on either side of the per|)endicu- F 2 08 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap lar line drawn from its starting point, and each sliot will liave a Prob: Error tliat we will call h. Do this for all the AA compartments in turn ; h will be the same for all of them, and the final result must be to re- produce the identically same system in the BB com- partments that was shown in Fig. 7, and in which each shot had a Prob: Error of q. The dispersion of the shot at BB may therefore be looked upon as compounded of two superimposed and independent systems of dispersion. In the one, when acting singly, each shot has a Prob: Error of a ; in the other, when acting singly, each shot has a Prob: Error of h, and the result of the two acting together is that each shot has a Prob: Error of q. What is the relation between a, h, and q ? Calculation shows that q^ = a^-\- h^. In other words, q corresponds to the hypo- thenuse of a right-angled triangle of which the other two sides are a and h respectively. (2) It is a coroUary of the foregoing that a system Z, in which each element is the Sum of a couple of inde- pendent Errors, of which one has been taken at random from a Normal system A and the other from a Normal system B, will itself be Normal.^ Calling the Q of the Z system q, and the Q of the A and B systems respectively, a and h, then g^ = a^ + h^. 1 We may see tlie rationale of this corollary if we invert part of the statement of the problem. Instead of saying that an a element deviates from its m, and that a b element also deviates independently from its m, we may phrase it thus : An a element deviates from its M, and its M deviates from the b element. Therefore the deviation of the b element from the A element is compounded of two independent deviations, as in Prohlem 1. v.] NORMAL VARIABILITY. 69 (3) Suppose that a row of compartments, whose upper openings are situated like those in Fig. 7, page 63, arc made first to converge towards some given point below, but that before reaching it their sloping course is checked and they are thenceforward allowed to drop vertically as in Fig. 9. The effect of this will be to compress the heaj) of shot laterally ; its outline will still be a Curve of Frequency, but its Prob: Error will be diminished. The foregoing three proj)erties of the Law of Error are well known to mathematicians and require no demon- stration here, but two other properties that are not familiar will be of use also ; proofs of them by ISir. J. Hamilton Dickson are given in Appendix B. They are as follows. I purposely select a different illustration to that used in the Appendix, for the sake of presenting the same general problem under more than one of its applications. (4) Bullets are fired by a man who aims at the centre of a target, which we will call its M, and we will suppose the marks that the bullets make to be painted red, for the sake of distinction. The system of lateral deviations of these red marks from the centre IVI will be approxi- mately Normal, whose Q we will call c. Then another man takes aim, not at the centre of the target, but at one or other of the red marks, selecting these at random. We will suppose his shots to be painted green. The lateral distance of any green shot from the red mark at which it was aimed will have a Prob: Error that we 70 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. v. will call h. Now, if the lateral distance of a particular green mark from IVI is given, what is the most prohahle distance from IVI of the red mark at which it was aimed ? (5) What is the Prob: Error of this determination ? In other words, if estimates have been made for a great many distances founded upon the formula in (4), they would be correct on the average, though erroneous in particular cases. The errors thus made would form a normal system whose Q it is desired to determine. Its 1 • he value IS -—— ^ Vip' + c^) By the help of these five problems the statistics of heredity become perfectly manageable. It will be found that they enable us to deal with Fraternities, Populations, or other Groups, just as if they were units. The largeness of the number of individuals in any of our groups is so far from scaring us, that they are actu- ally welcomed as making the calculations more sure and none the less simple. CHAPTEE VI. DATA. EecorJs of rainilj Faculties, or E. F. F. data. — Special Data. — Measures at my AntLropometric Laboratory. — Experiments on Sweet Peas. I HAD to collect all my data for myself, as nothing existed, so far as I know, that would satisfy even my primary requirement. This was to obtain records of at least two successive generations of some population of considerable size. They must have lived under con- ditions that were of a usual kind, and in which no great varieties of nurture were to be found. Natural selection must have had little influence on the characteristics that were to be examined. These must be measurable, variable, and fairly constant in the same individual. The result of numerous inquiries, made of the most competent persons, was that I began my experiments many years ago on the seeds of sweet peas, and that at the present time I am breeding moths, as will be explained in a later chapter, but this book refers to a human population, which, take it all in all, is the easiest to work with when the data are once ol:>tained, 72 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. to say notliing of its being more interesting by far than one of sweet peas or of moths. Record of Family Faculties, or R.F.F. Data. — The source from which the larger part of my data is derived consists of a valuable collection of " Kecords of Family Faculties," obtained through the offer of prizes. They have been much tested and cross-tested, and have borne the ordeal very fairly, so far as it has been applied. It is well to reprint the terms of the published offer, in order to give a just idea of the conditions under which they were compiled. It was as follows : '' Mr. Francis Galton offers 500^. in prizes to those British Subjects resident in the United Kingdom who shall furnish him before May 15, 1884, with the best Extracts from theii* own Family Eecords. " These Extracts will be treated as confidential docu- ments, to be used for statistical purposes only, the insertion of names of persons and places being required solely as a guarantee of authenticity and to enable Mr. Galton to communicate with the writers in cases where further question may be necessary. " The value of the Extracts will be estimated by the degree in which they seem likely to facilitate the scien- tific investigations described in the preface to the 'Eecord of Family Faculties.' " More especially : " (a) By including every direct ancestor who stands within the limits of kinship there specified. " (6) By including brief notices of the brothers and vl] data. 73 sisters (if any) of each of tliose ancestors. (Importance will be attached both to the completeness with which each family of brothers and sisters is described, and also to the number of persons so described.) " (c) By the character of the evidence upon which the information is based. " (d) By the clearness and conciseness with Y/hich the statements and remarks are made. " The Extracts must be legibly entered either in the tabular forms contained in the copy of the ' Record of Family Faculties ' (into which, if more space is wanted, additional pages may be stitched), or they may be written in any other book with pages of the same size as those of the Record, provided that the information be arranged in the same tabular form and order. (It will be obvious that uniformity in the arrangement of docu- ments is of primary importance to those who examine and collate a large number of them.) " Each competitor must furnish the name and address of a referee of good social standing (magistrate, clergy- man, lawyer, medical practitioner, &c.), who is personally acquainted with his family, and of whom inquiry may be made, if desired, as to the general trustworthiness of the competitor. " The Extracts must be sent prepaid and by post, addressed to Francis Galton, 42 Rutland Gate, London, S.W. It will be convenient if the letters ' R.F.F.' (Record of Family Faculties) be written in the left- hand corner of the parcel, below the address. 74 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. " The examination will be conducted by tlie donor of tlie prizes, aided by competent examiners. " The value of the individual prizes cannot be fixed beforehand. No prize will, however, exceed 5.0/., nor be less than 5l., and 500/. will on the whole be awarded. '' A list of the gainers of the prizes will be posted to each of them. It will be published in one or more of the daily newspapers, also in at least one clerical, and one medical Journal." The number of Family Eecords sent in reply to this offer, that deserved to be seriously considered before adjudging the prizes, barely reached 150 ; 70 of these being contributed by males, 80 by females. The re- mainder were imperfect, or they were marked " not for competition," but at least 10 of these have been to some degree utilised. The 150 Records were contributed by persons of very various ranks. After classing the female writers according to the profession of their husbands, if they were married, or according to that of their fathers, if they were unmarried, I found that each of the following 7 classes had 20 or somewhat fewer representatives : (l) Titled persons and landed gentry ; (2) Army and Navy ; (3) Church (various denomina- tions) ; (4) Law ; (5) Medicine ; (6) Commerce, higher class ; (7) Commerce, lower class. This accounts for nearly 130 of the writers of the Eecords ; the remainder are land agents, farmers, artisans, literary men, school- masters, clerks, students, and one domestic servant in a family of position. VI.] DATA. 75 Three cases occurred in which the Records sent by different contributors overlapped. The details are complicated, and need not be described here, but the result is that five persons have been adjudged smaller prizes than they individually deserved. Every one of the replies refers to a very large number of persons, as will easily be understood if the fact is borne in mind that each individual has 2 parents, 4 grandparents, and 8 great parents; also that he and each of those 14 progenitors had usually brothers and sisters, who were included in the incjuiry. The replies were unequal in merit, as might have been expected, but many were of so high an order that I could not justly select a few as recipients of large prizes to the exclusion of the rest. Therefore I divided the sum into two considerable groups of small prizes, all of which were well deserved, regretting much that I had none left to award to a few others of nearly ecjual merit to some of those who had been successful. The list of winners is reproduced below, the four years that have elapsed have of course made not a few changes in the addresses, which are not noticed here. LIST OF AWAEDS. A Prize of £1 was awarded to each op the 40 following Contributors. Amphlett, Jolin, Clent, Stourbridge ; Batchelor, Mrs. Jacobstow Kectory, Stratton, N. Devon ; Bathnrst, Miss K., Vicarage, Biggleswade, Bedford- sliire ; Beane, Mrs. C. F., 3 Portland Place, Venner Eoad, Sydenham ; Berisford, Samuel, Park Villas, Park Lane, Macclesfield ; Carruthers, Mrs., Briglitside, North Finchley ; Carter, Miss Jessie E., Hazelwood, The Park, Cheltenham ; Cay, Mrs., Eden House, Holyhead ; Clark, J. Edmund, 76 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. I'eversham Terrace, York ; Oust, Lady Elizabetli, 13 Eccleston Square, S.W. ; Fry, Edward, Portsmouth, 5 The Grove, Highgate, N. ; Gibson, G. A., M D., 1 Eandolph Cliff, Edinburgh ; Gidley, B. Conrtenay, 17 Ribblesdale Road, Hornsey, N. ; Gillespie, Franklin, M.D., 1 The Grove, Aldershot ; Griffith- Boscawen, Mrs., Trevalyn Hall, AVrexham ; Hard castle, Henry, 38 Eaton Square, S.W. ; Harrison, Miss Edith, 68 Gloucester Place, Portman Square, W. ; Hobhouse, Mrs. 4 Kensington Square, W. ; Holland, INIiss, Ivynieath, Snodland, Kent ; Ilollis, George, Dartmouth House, Dartmouth Park Hill, N. ; Ingram, Mrs. Ades, Chailey, Lewis, Sussex ; Johnstone, Miss C. L., 3 Clarendon Place, Leamington ; Lane-Poole, Stanley, 6 Park Villas East, Richmond, Middlesex ; Leathley, D. W. B., 59 Lincoln's Inn Fields (in trast for a competitor who desires her name not to be published) ; Lewin, Lieutenant-Colonel T. H., Colway Lodge, Lyme Regis ; Lipscomb, R. H., East Buclleigh, Budleigh Salterton, Devon ; Maiden, Henry C, Wincllesham House, Brighton ; Maiden, Henry Elliot, Kitland, Holmwood, Surrey ; McCall, Hardy Bertram, 5 St. Augustine's Road, Edgbaston, Birmingham ; Moore, Miss Georgina M., 45 Chepstow Place, Bayswaler, AV. ; Newlands, Mrs., Raeden, near Aberdeen ; Pearson, David R., M.D., 23 Upper Phili- more Place, Kensington, W. ; Pearson, Mrs., The Garth, Woodside Park, North Finchley : Pechell, Hervey Charles, 6 West Chapel Street, Curzon Street, W. ; Roberts, Samuel, 21 Roland Gardens, S.W. ; Smith, Mrs. Archibald, Riverbank, Putney, S.W. ; Strachey, Mrs. Fowey Lodge, Clapham Common, S.W. ; Sturge, Miss Mary C, Chilliswood, Tyndall's Park, Bristol ; Sturge, Mrs. R. F., 101 Pembroke Road, Clifton ; Wilson, Edward T., M.D., Westall, Cheltenham. A Prize of £b was awarded to each of the 44 following Contributors. Allan, Francis J., M.D., 1 Dock Street, E. ; Atkinson, Mrs., Clare College Lodge, Cambridge ; Bevan, Mrs , Plumpton House, Bury St. Edmunds ; Browne, Miss, Maidenwell House, Louth, Lincolnshire ; Cash, Frederick Goodall, Gloucester ; Chisholm, Mrs., Church Lane House, Haslemere, Surrey ; Collier, Mrs. R., 7 Thames Embankment, Chelsea ; Croft, Sir Herbert G. D., Lugwardine Court, Hereford ; Davis, Mrs. (care of Israel Davis, 6 King's Bench Walk, Temple, E.G.) ; Drake, Henry H., The Firs, Lee, Kent; Ercke, J. J. G., 13, Brownhill Road, Catford, S.E. ; Flint, Fenner Ludd, 83 Brecknock Eoad, N. ; Ford, William, 4 South Square, Gray's Inn, W.C. ; Foster, Rev. A. J., The Vicarage, Wootton, Bedford ; Glanville-Richards, AV. V. S., 23 Endsleigh Place, Plymouth ; Hale, C. D. Bowditch, 8 Sussex Gardens, Hyde Park, W. ; Horder, Mrs. Mark, Rothenwood, Ellen Grove, Salisbury ; Jackson, Edwin, 79 Withington Road, Whalley Range, Manchester ; Jackson, George, 1 St. George's Terrace, Plymouth ; Kesteven, W. H., 401 Holloway Road, N. ; Lawrence, Mrs. VI.] DATA. 77 Alfred, 16 Suffolk Square, Cheltenham ; Lawrie, Mrs., 1 Chesham Place, S.W. ; Leveson-Gower, G. W. G., Titsey Place, Limpsfield, Surrey ; Lobb, H. W., 66 Eussell Square, W. ; McConnell, Miss M. A. Brooklands, Prestwich, Manchester ; Marshall, Mrs., Fenton Hall, Stoke-upon-Trent ; Meyer, Mrs., 1 Rodney Place, Clifton, Bristol; Milman, Mrs., The Governor's House, H.M. Prison, Camden Road ; Olding, Mrs. W. 4 Brunswick Road, Brighton, Sussex ; Passingham, Mrs., Milton, Cambridge ; Pringle, Mrs. Fairnalie, Fox Grove Road, Beckenham, Kent ; Reeve, Miss, Foxholes, Christchurch, Hants ; Scarlett, Mrs., Boscorab Manor, Bournemouth ; Shand, William, 57 Caledonian Road, N. ; Shaw, Cecil E., Wellington Park, Belfast ; Sizer, Miss Kate T., Moorlands, Great Huntley, Colchester ; Smith, Miss A. M. Carter, Thistleworth, Stevenage ; Smith, Rev. Edward S., Yiney Hall Vicarage, Blakeney, Gloucestershire ; Smith, Mrs. F, P., Cliffe House, Sheffield ; Staveley, Edw. S. R., Mill Hill School, N.W. ; Slurge, Miss Mary W., 17 Frederick Road, Eclgbaston, Birmingham ; Terry, Mrs., Tostock, Bury St. Edmunds, Suffolk; Utley, W, H. Alliance Hotel, Cathedral Gates, Manchester ; Weston, Mrs. Ensleigh, Lansdown, Bath ; Wodehouse, Mrs. E. R. 50 Chester Square, S.W. The material in these Eecords is sufficiently varied to be of service in many inquiries. The chief subjects to which allusion will be made in this book concern Statnre, Eye-Colonr, Temper, the Artistic Faculty, and some forms of Disease, but others are utilized that refer to Marriage Selection and Fertility. The following remarks in this Chapter refer almost wholly to the data of Stature. The data derived from the Eecords of Family Faculties will be hereafter distinguished by the letters R.F.F. I was able to extract from them the statures of 205 couples of parents, with those of an aggregate of 930 of their adult children of both sexes. I must repeat that when dealing with the female statures, I transmuted them to their male equivalents ; and treated them when thus transmuted, on equal terms with the measures of males. 78 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. except where otherwise expressed. The factor I used was 1*08, which is equivalent to adding a little less than one-twelfth to each female height. It differs slightly from the factors employed by other anthropologists, who, moreover, differ a trifle between themselves ; any- how, it suits my data better than 1*07 or 1*09. I can say confidently that the final result is not of a kind to be sensibly affected by these minute details, because it happened that owing to a mistaken direction, the com- puter to whom I first entrusted the figures used a somewhat different factor, yet the final results came out closely the same. These E.F.F. data have by no means the precision of the observations to be spoken of in the next paragraph. In many cases there remains consider- able doubt whether the measurement refers to the height with the shoes on or off; not a few of the entries are, I fear, only estimates, and the heights are commonly given only to the nearest inch. StiU, speaking from a know- ledge of many of the contributors, I am satisfied that a fair share of these returns are undoubtedly careful and thoroughly trustworthy, and as there is no sign or sus- picion of bias, I have reason to place confidence in the values of the Means that are derived, from them. They bear the internal tests that have been appHed better than might have been expected, and when checked by the data described in the next paragraph, and cautiously treated, they are very valuable. Special Data. — A second set of data, distinguished by the name of " Sj^ecial observations," concern the VI.] DATA. 79 variations in stature amona; Brothers. I circulated cards of inquiry among trusted correspondents, stating that I wanted records of the heights of brothers who were more than 24 and less than 60 years of age ; that it was not necessary to send the statures of all of the brothers of the same family, but only of as many of them as could be easily and accurately measured, and that the height of even two brothers would be acceptable. The blank forms sent to be filled, were ruled vertically in three parallel columns : (a) family name of each set of brothers ; (b) order of birth in each set ; (c) height without shoes, in feet and inches. A place was reserved at the bottom for the name and address of the sender. The circle of inquirers widened, but I was satisfied when I had obtained returns of 295 families, containing in the aggregate 783 brothers, some few of whom also appear in the E.F.F. data. Though these two sets of returns overlap to a trifling extent, they are practically independent. I look upon the " Special Observations " as being quite as trustworthy as could be expected in any such returns. They bear every internal test that I can apply to them in a very satisfactory manner. The mea- sures are commonly recorded to quarter or half inches. Measures at my Anthropometric Laboratory. — A third set of data have been incidentally of service. They are the large lists of measures, nearly 10,000 in number, made at my Anthropometric Laboratory in the International Health Exhibition of 1884. 4. Experiments on Sweet Peas. — The last of the data 80 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. that I need specify were the very first that I used ; they refer to the sizes of seeds, which are equivalent to the Statures of seeds. I both measured and weighed them, but after assuring myself of the equivalence of the two methods (see Appendix C), confined myself to ascertaining the weights, as they were much more easily ascertained than the measures. It is more than 10 years since I procured these data. They were the result of an extensive series of experiments on the produce of seeds of difi'erent sizes, . but of the same species, conducted for the following reasons. I had endeavoured to find a population possessed of some measurable characteristic that was suitable for investigating the causes of the statistical similarity between successive generations of a people, as will here- after be discussed in Chapter YIII. At last I determined to experiment on seeds, and after much inquiry of very competent advisers, selected sweet-peas for the purpose. They do not cross-fertilize, which is a very exceptional condition among plants ; they are hardy, prolific, of a convenient size to handle, and nearly spherical ; their weight does not alter perceptibly when the air changes from damp to dry, and the little pea at the end of the pod, so characteristic of ordinary peas, is absent in sweet- peas. I began by weighing thousands of them individ- ually, and treating them as a census ofiicer would treat a large population. Then I selected with great pains several sets for planting. Each set contained seven little packets, numbered K, L, M, N, 0, P, and Q, each of the seven packets contained ten seeds of almost VI.] DATA. 81 exactly the same weight ; those in K being the heaviest, L the next heaviest, and so down to Q, which was the lightest. The precise weights are given in Appendix C, together with the corresponding diameters, which I ascertained by laying 100 peas of the same weight in a row. The weights^ rim in an arithmetic series, ha^dng a common average difference of 0*172 grain. I do not of course profess to work to thousandths of a grain, though I did work to somewhat less than one hundredth of a grain ; therefore the third decimal place represents little more than an arithmetical working value which has to be regarded in multiplications, lest an error of sensible im- portance should be introduced by its neglect. Curiously enough, the diameters were found also to run approxi- mately in an arithmetic series, owing, I suppose, to the misshape and corrugations of the smaller seeds, which gave them a larger diameter than if they had been plumped out into spheres. AH this is shown in the Appendix, where it will be seen that T was justified in sorting the seeds by the convenient method of the balance and weights, and of accepting the weights as directly proportional to the mean diameters. In each experiment, seven beds were prepared in parallel rows ; each was 1^ feet wide and 5 feet long. Ten holes of 1 inch deep were dibbled at equal distances apart along each bed, and a single seed was put into each hole. The beds were then bushed over to keep off the birds. Minute instructions were given to ensure uniformity, which I need not repeat here. The end of all was that the seeds as they became ripe were G 82 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. vi. collected from time to time and put into bags that I had sent, lettered from K to Q, the same letters having been stuck at the ends of the beds. When the crop was coming to an end, the whole remaining produce of each bed, including the foliage, was torn up, tied together, labelled, and sent to me. Many friends and acquaint- ances had each undertaken the planting and culture of a complete set, so that I had simultaneous experiments going on in various parts of the United Kingdom from Nairn in the North to Cornwall in the South. Two proved failures, but the final result was that I obtained the more or less complete produce of seven sets ; that is to say, the produce of 7x7x10, or of 490 carefully weighed parent seeds. Some additional account of the results is given in Appendix C. It would be wholly out of place to enter here into further details of the experiments, or to narrate the numerous little difiiculties and imperfections I had to contend with, and how I balanced doubtful cases ; how I divided returns into groups to see if they confirmed one another, or how I conducted any other w^ell-known statistical operation. Sufiice it to say that I took im- mense pains, which, if I had understood the general conditions of the ^^I'o^^lem as clearly as I do now, I should not perhaps have cared to bestow. The results were most satisfactory. They gave me two data, which were all that I wanted in order to understand in its simplest approximate form, the way in which one generation of a people is descended from a previous one ; and thus I got at the heart of the problem at once. CHAPTER YIL DISCUSSION OF THE DATA OF STATUTE. Stature as a subject for inquiry, — Marriage Selection. — Issue of unlike Parents. — Description of the Tables of Stature. Mid-Stature of tbe Population. — Variability of the Population. — Variability of Mid- Parents. — Variability in Co-Fraternities. — Regression : «, Filial ; 6, Mid-Parental ; c, Parental ; d^ Fraternal. — Squadrons of Statures. — Successive Generations of a People. — Natural Selection. — Variability in Fraternities. — Trustworthiness of tlie Constants. — General view of Kinship. — Separate Contribution from each Ancestor. — Pedigree Lloths. Stature as a Subject for Inquiry. — The first of these inquiries into the laws of human heredity deals with hereditary Stature, which is an excellent subject for statistics. Some of its merits are obvious enough, such as the ease and frequency with which it may be measured, its practical constancy during thirty-five or forty years of middle life, its comparatively small dependence upon clifi'erences of bringing up, and its inconsiderable influ- ence on the rate of mortalitv. Other advantaQ;es which are not equally obvious are equally great. One of these is due to the fact that human stature is not a simple element, but a sum of the accumidated lengths or G 2 Si NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. thicknesses of more than a hnndred iDoclily parts, each so distinct from the rest as to have earned a name by which it can be s|)ecified. The list includes about fifty separate bones, situated in the skull, the spine, the pelvis, the two legs, and in the two ankles and feet. The bones in both the lower limbs have to be counted, because the Stature depends upon their average length. The two cartilages interposed between adjacent bones, wherever there is a movable joint, and the single cartilage in other cases, are rather more numerous than the bones themselves. The fleshy parts of the scalp of the head and of the soles of the feet conclude the list Account should also be taken of the shape and set of the many bones which conduce to a more or less arched instep, straight back, or high head. I noticed in the skeleton of O'Brien, the Irish giant, at the College of Surgeons, which is the tallest skeleton in any English museum, that his great stature of about 7 feet 7 inches would have been a trifle increased if the faces of his dorsal vertebrae had been more parallel than they are, and his back consequently straighter. This multiplicity of elements, whose variations are to some degree independent of one another, some tending to lengthen the total stature, others to shorten it, corresponds to an equal number of sets of rows of pins in the apparatus Fig. 7, p. 63, by which the cause of variability was illustrated. The larger the number of these variable elements, the more nearly does the varia- bility of their sum assume a "Normal" character, though the approximation increases only as the sc[uare root of VII.J DISCUSSION OF THE DATA OF STATURE. 85 their number. The beautiful regularity in the Statures of a population, whenever they are statistically marshalled in the order of their heights, is due to the number of variable and quasi-independent elements of which Stature is the sum. Mai^riage Selection. — Whatever may be the sexual preferences for similarity or for contrast, I find little indication in the average results obtained from a fairly large number of cases, of any single measurable personal peculiarity, whether it be stature, temper, eye-colour, or artistic tastes, in influencing marriage selection to a notable degree. Nor is this extraordinary, for though people may fall in love for trifles, marriage is a serious act, usually determined by the concurrence of numerous motives. Therefore we could hardly expect either shortness or tallness, darkness or lightness in com- plexion, or any other single quality, to have in the long run a large separate influence. I was certainly surprised to find how imperceptible was the influence that even good and bad Temper seemed to exert on marriage selection. A list was made (see Appendix D) of the observed frequency of marriages between persons of each of the various classes of Temper, in a group of 111 couples, and I calculated what would have been the relative frequency of intermarriages be- tween persons of the various classes, if the same number of males and females had been paired at random. The result showed that the observed list agreed closely with the calculated list, and therefore that these observations 86 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. gave no evidence of discriminative selection in respect to Temper. The good-tempered husbands were 46 per cent, in number, and, between them, they married 22 good-tempered and 24 bad-tempered wives ; whereas calculation, having regard to the relative proportions of good and bad TemjDer in the two sexes, gave the numbers as 25 and 21. Again, the bad-tempered hus- bands, who were 54 per cent, in number, married 31 good-tempered and 23 bad-tempered wives, whereas calculation gave the number as 30 and 24. This rough summary is a just expression of the results arrived at by a more minute analysis, which is described in the Appendix, and need not be repeated here. Similarly as regards Eye-Colour. If we analyse the marriages between the 78 couples whose eye-colours are described in Chapter YIIL, and compare the observed results with those calculated on the supposition that Eye-Colour has no influence whatever in marriage selection, the two lists will be found to be much alike. Thus where both of the parents have eyes of the same colour, whether they be light, or hazel, or dark, the percentage results are almost identical, being 37, 3, and 8 as observed, against 37, 2, and 7 calculated. Where one parent is hazel-eyed and the other dark-eyed, the marriages are as 5 observed against 7 calculated. But the results run much less well together in the other two possible combinations, for where one parent is light and the other hazel-eyed, they give 23 observed against 15 calculated ; and where one parent is light and the other dark-eyed, they give 24 observed against 32 calculated. VII.] DISCUSSION OF THE DATA OF STATURE. 87 The effect of Artistic Taste on marriage selection is discussed in Chapter X., and this also is shown to be small. The influence on the race of Bias in Marriage Selection will be discussed in that chapter. I have taken much trouble at different times to determine whether Stature plays any sensible part in marriage selection. I am not yet prepared to offer complete results, but shall confine my remarks for the present to the particular cases with which we are now concerned. The shrewdest test is to proceed under the guidance of Problem 2, page 68. I find the Q of Stature among the male population to be 1*7 inch, and similarly for the transmuted statures of the female population. Consequently if the men and (transmuted) women married at random so far as stature was con- cerned, the Q in a group of couples, each couple consisting of a pair of summed statures, would be \/2i X 1"7 inches ==2*41 inches. Therefore the Q in a group of which each element is the mean stature of a couple, would be half that amount, or 1'20 inch. This closely corresponds to what I derived from the data contained in the first and in the last column but one of Table 11. The word " Mid-Parent," in the headings to those columns, expresses an ideal person of composite sex, whose Stature is half way between the Stature of the father and the transmuted Stature of the mother. I therefore conclude that marriage selection does not pay such regard to Stature, as deserves being taken into account in the cases with which we are concerned. I tried the question in another but ruder way, l^y 88 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. dividing (see Table 9) the male and female parents re- spectively into three nearly equal groups, of tall, medium, and short. It was impracticable to make them precisely equal, on account of the roughness with Avhich the measurements were recorded, so I framed rules that seemed best adapted to the case. Consequently the numbers of the tall and short proved to be only ap- proximately and not exactly equal, and the two together were only approximately equal to the medium cases. The final results were : — 32 instances where one parent was short and the other tall, and 27 where both were short or both were tall. In other words, there were 32 cases of contrast in marriage, to 27 cases of likeness. I do not regard this difference as of consequence, because the numbers are small, and because a slight change in the limiting values assigned to shortness and tallness, would have a sensible effect upon the result. I am therefore content to ignore it, and to regard the Statures of married folk just as if their choice in mar- riage had been wholly independent of stature. The importance of this supposition in facilitating calculation will be appreciated as we proceed. Issue of Unlike Parents. — AVe will next discuss the question whether the Stature of the issue of unlike parents betrays any notable evidence of their unlikeness, or whether the peculiarities of the children do not rather depend on the average of two values ; one the Stature of the father, and the other the transmuted Stature of the mother ; in other words, on the Stature of VII.] DISCLTSICN CF THE DATA OF STATURE. 89 that ideal personage to wliom we have akeady been introduced under the name of a Mid- Parent. Stature has already been spoken of as a well-marked instance of the heritages that blend freely in the course of hereditary transmission. It now becomes necessary to substantiate the statement, because it is proposed to trace the relationship between the Mid-Parent and the Son. It would not be possible to discuss the relationship between either parent singly, and the son, in a trust- w^orthy way, without the help of a much larger number of observations than are now at my disposal. They ought to be numerous enough to give good assurance that the casea of tall and short, among the unknown parents, shall neutralise one another ; otherwise the uncertainty of the stature of the unknown parent would make the re- sults uncertain to a serious degree. I am heartily glad that I shall be able fully to justify the method of deal- ing with Mid-Parentages instead of with single Parents. The evidence is as follows : — If the Stature of children depends only upon the average Stature of their two Parents, that of the mother having been first trans- muted, it will make no difi'erence in a Fraternity whether one of the Parents was tall and the other short, or whether they were alike in Stature. But if some children resemble one Parent in Stature and others resemble the other, the Fraternity will be more diverse when their Parents had differed in Stature than when they were alike. We easily acquaint ourselves with the facts by separating a considerable number of Fraternities into two contrasted groups : (a) those who are the progeny 90 NATURAL INHERITANCE. ^cnAP. of Like Parents ; (b) those who are tlie progeny of Unlike Parents. Next we write tlie statures of the individuals in eacli Fraternity under the form of M + (dzD) (see page 51), where M is the mean stature of the Fraternity, and D is the deviation of any one of its members from M. Then we marshal all the values of D that belong to the group a, into one Scheme of de^dations, and all those that belong to the group h into another Scheme, and we find the Q of each. If it should be the same, then there is no greater diversity in the a Grroup than there is in the b Group, and such proves to be the case. I applied the test (see Table 10) to a total of 525 children, and found that they were no more diverse in the one case than in the other. I therefore conclude that we have only to look to the Stature of the Mid-Parent, and need not care whether the Parents are or are not unlike one another. The advantages of Stature as a subject from which the simple laws of heredity may be studied, will now be well appreciated. It is nearly constant in the same adult, it is frequently measured and recorded ; its dis- cussion need not be entangled with considerations of marriage selection. It is sufficient to consider the Stature of the Mid-Parent and not those of the two Parents separately. Its variability is Normal, so that much use may be made of the curious properties of the law of Frequency of Error in cross-testing the several con- clusions, and I may add that in all cases they have borne the test successfully. VII.] DISCUSSION OF THE DATA OF STATURE. 91 The only drawback to tlie use of Stature in statistical inquiries, is its small variability, one balf of tbe popula- tion differing less than 1'7 inch, from the average of all of tliem. In other words, its Q is only 1'7 inch. Description of the ToMes of Stature. — I have arranged and discussed my materials in a great variety of ways, to guard against rash conclusions, but do not think it necessary to trouble the reader with more than a few Tables, which afford sufficient material to determine the more important constants in the formulae that will be used. Table 11, E.F.F., refers to the relation between the Mid-Parent and his (or should we say its ?) Sons and Transmuted Daughters, and it records the Statures of 928 adult offspring of 205 Mid-Parents. It shows the distribution of Stature among the Sons of each succes- sive group of Mid-Parents, in which ^ the latter are all ~of the same Stature, reckoning to the nearest inch. I have calculated the M of each line, chiefly by drawing Schemes from the entries in it. Their values are printed at the ends of the lines and they form the right-hand column of the Table. Tables 12 and 13 refer to the relation between Brothers. The one is derived frcm the E.F.F. and the other from the Special data. They both deal with small or moder- ately sized Fraternities, excluding the larger ones for reasons that will be explained directly, but the E.F.F. Table is the least restricted in this respect, as it only excludes families of 6 brothers and upwards. The data 92 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. were so few in number tliat I could not well afford to lop off more. These Tables were constructed by registering the differences between each possible pair of brothers in each family : thus if there were three brothers, A, B, and C, in a particular family, I entered the differences of stature between A and B, A and C, and B and C, four brothers gave rise to 6 entries, and five brothers to 10 entries. The larger Fraternities were omitted, as the very large number of different pairs in them would have overwhelmed the influence of the smaller Frater- nities. Large Fraternities are separately dealt with in Table 14. We can derive some of the constants by more than one method ; and it is gratifying to find how well the results of different methods confirm one another. Mid-Stature of the Population. — The Median, Mid- Stature, or M of 'the general Population is a value of primary importance in this inquiry. Its value will be always designated by the symbol P, and it may be deduced from the bottom lines of any one of the three Tables. I obtain from them respectively the values 6 8 '2, 68 '5, 68 '4, but the middle of these, which is printed in italics, is a smoothed result. It is one of the only two smoothed values in the whole of my work, and was justifiably corrected, because the observed values that happen to lie nearest to the Grade of 50° ran out of harmony with the rest of the curve. It is therefore reasonable to consider its discrepancy as fortuitous, althouQ-h it amounts to more than 0'15 inch. The VII.] DISCUSSION OF THE DATA OF STATURE. 93 series in question refers to E.F.F. brothers, wlio, owing to the principle on whicli the Table is constructed, are only a comparatively small sample taken out of the E.F.F. Population, and on a principle that gave greater weight to a few large families than to all the rest. Therefore it could not be expected to give rise to so regular a Scheme for the general E.F.F. Population as Table 11, which was fairly based upon the whole of it. Less accuracy was undoubtedly to have been expected in this group than in either of the others. Variahility of the Population. — The value of Q in the Statures of the general Population is to be deduced from the bottom lines of any one of the Tables 11, 12, and 13. The three values of it that I so obtain, are 1'65, 1*7, and 1'7 inch. I should mention that the method of the treatment originally adopted, happened also to make the first of these values 1 '7 inch, so I have no hesitation in accepting 1*7 as the value for all my ciaLa. Variability of Mid-Parents. — The value of Q in a Scheme drawn from the Statures of the E.F.F. Mid- Parents according to the data in Table 11, is 1'19 inches. Now it has already been shown that if marriage selection is independent of stature, the value of Q in the Scheme of Mid-parental Statures would be ec[ual to its value in that of the general Population (which we have just seen to be 1*7 inch), divided by the square root of 2 ; that is by 1'45. This calculation makes it to be 94 NATURAL INHERITANCE. . [chap. 1*21 iiicli, which agrees excellently with the observed value/ Variahilky in Co- Fraternities. — As all the Adult Sons and Transmuted Daughters of the same Mid- Parent, form what is called a Fraternity, so all the Adult Sons and Transmuted Daughters of a group of Mid- Parents who have the same Stature (reckoned to the nearest inch) will be termed a Co-Fraternity. Each line in Table 11 refers to a separate Co-Fraternity and expresses the distribution of Stature among them. There are three reasons why Co-Fraternals should be more diverse among themselves than brothers. First, because their Mid-Parents are not of identical height, but may differ even as much as one inch. Secondly, because their grandparents, great-grandparents, and so on indefinitely backwards, may have difi'ered widely. Thirdly, because the nurture or rearing of Co-Fraternals is more various than that of Fraternals. The brothers in a Fraternity of townsfolk do not seem to difi'er more among themselves than those in a Fraternity of country- folk, but a mixture of Fraternities derived indiscrimi- nately from the two sources, must show greater diversity than either of them taken by themselves. The large difi'erences between town and country-folk, and those between persons of difi'erent social classes, are con- spicuous in the data contained in the Report of the '^ In all my values referring to liuman stature, tlie second decimal is rudely approximate. I am obliged to use it, because if I worked only to tentlis of an incli, sensible errors might creep in entirely owing to aritli- metical operations. vii.] DISCUSSION OF THE DATA OF STATURE. 95 Anthropological Committee to the British Association in 1880, and published in its Journal. I concluded after carefully studying the chart upon which each of the individual observations from which Table 11 was constructed, had been entered separately in their appropriate places, and not clubbed into groups as in the Tables, that the value of Q in each Co- Fraternal group was roughly the same, whatever their Mid-Parental value might have been. It was not cjuite the same, being a trifle larger when the Mid-Parents were tall than when they were short. This justifies what will be said in Appendix E about the G-eometric Mean ; it also justifies neglect in the present inquiry of the method founded upon it, because the improvement in the results to which it might lead, would be insignifi- cant, while its use would have added to the difiiculty of explanation, and introduced extra trouble throuo-h- out, to the reader more than to myself. The value that I adopt for Q in every Co-Fraternal group, is 1*5 inch. Regression. — a. Filial : However paradoxical it may appear at first sight, it is theoretically a necessary fact, and one that is clearly confirmed by observation, that the Stature of the adult ofi*spring must on the whole, be more mediocre than the stature of their Parents ; that is to say, more near to the IVI of the general Population. Table 11 enables us to compare the values of the IVI in difi'erent Co-Fraternal groups with the Statures of their respective Mid-Parents. Fig. 10 is a graphical representation of the meaning of 96 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. the Table so far as it now concerns ns. The horizontal dotted lines and the graduations at their sides, cor- respond to the similarly placed lines of figures and graduations in Table 11. The dot on each line shows the point where its IVI falls. The value of its M is to be read on the graduations along the top, and is the same as that which is given in the last column of Table 11. It will be perceived that the line drawn FIG.EO. through the centres of the dots, admits of being inter- preted by the straight line C D, with but a small amount of give and take ; and the fairness of this interpretation is confirmed by a study of the MS. chart above mentioned, in which the individual observations were plotted in their right places. Now if we draw a line A B through every point where the graduations along the top of Fig. 10, are the same as those along the sides, the line will be straight and. will run diagonally. It represents what the Mid- VII.] DISCUSSION OF THE DATA OF STATURE. 97 Statures of the Sons would be, if tliey were on tlie average identical with those of their Mid-Parents. Most obviously A B does not agree with C D ; therefore Sons do not, on the average, resemble their Mid- Parents. On examining these lines more closely, it will be observed that A B cuts C D at a point M that fairly corresponds to the value of 685- inches, whether its value be read on the scale at the top or on that at the side. This is the value of P, the Mid-Stature of the population. Therefore it is only when the Parents are mediocre, that their Sons on the average resemble them. Next draw a vertical line, E M F, through M, and let EGA be any horizontal line cutting ME at E, MC at E, and MA at A. Then it is obvious that the ratio of EA to EC is constant, whatever may be the position of E C A. This is true whether E C A be drawn above or like F D B, below M. In other words, the proportion between the Mid-Filial and the Mid-Parental deviation is constant, whatever the Mid-Parental stature may be. I reckon this ratio to be as 2 to 3 : that is to say, the Filial deviation from P is on the average only two- thirds as wide as the Mid-Parental Deviation. I call this ratio of 2 to 3 the ratio of " Filial Eegression." It is the proportion in which the Son is, on the average, less exceptional than his Mid-Parent. My first estimate of the average proportion between the Mid-Filial and the Mid-Parental deviations, was made from a study of the MS. chart, and I then reckoned it as 3 to 5. The value given above was H 98 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. afterwards substituted, because the data seemed to admit of that interpretation also, in which case the fraction of two-thirds was preferable as being the more simple expression. I am now inclined to think the latter may be a trifle too small, but it is not worth while to make alterations until a new, larger, and more accurate series of observations can be discussed, and the whole work revised. The present doubt only ranges between nine-fifteenths in the first case and ten- fifteenths in the second. This value of two-thirds will therefore be accepted as the amount of Eegression, on the average of many cases, from the Mid-Parental to the Mid-Filial stature, whatever the Mid-Parental stature may be. As the two Parents contribute equally, the contribu- tion of either of them can be only one half of that of the two jointly ; in other words, only one half of that of the Mid-Parent. Therefore the average Eegression from the Parental to the Mid-Filial Stature must be the one half of two-thirds, or one-third. I am unable to test this conclusion in a satisfactory manner by direct observation. The data are barely numerous enough for dealing even with questions referring to Mid- Parentages ; they are quite insufficient to deal with those that involve the additional large uncertainty introduced owing to an ignorance of the Stature of one of the parents. I have entered the Uni-Parental and the Filial data on a MS. chart, each in its appropriate place, but they are too scattered and irregular to make it useful to give VII.] DISCUSSION OF THE DATA OF STATURE. 99 the results in detail. They seem to show a Eegression of about two-fifths, which differs from that of one-third in the ratio of 6 to 5. This direct observation is so inferior in value to the inferred result, that I disregard it, and am satisfied to adopt the value given by the latter, that is to say, of one-third, to express the averag^e Recession from either of the Parents to the Son. h. Mid-Parental : The converse relation to that which we have just discussed, namely the relation between the unknown stature of the Mid-Parent and the known Stature of the Son, is expressed by a fraction that is very far from being the converse of two-thirds. Though the Son deviates on the average from P only f as widely as his Mid-parent, it does not in the least follow that the Mid-parent should deviate on the average from P, 1^ or 1-J-, as widely as the Son. The Mid-Parent is not likely to be more exceptional than the son, but quite the contrary. The number of individuals who are nearly mediocre is so preponderant, that an ex- ceptional man is more frequently found to be the exceptional son of mediocre parents than the average son of very exceptional parents. This is clearly shown by Table 1 1 , where the very same observations which give the average value of Filial Regression when it is read in one way, gives that of the Mid-Parental Regression when it is read in another way, namely down the vertical columns, instead of along the horizontal lines. It then shows that the Mid-Parent of a man deviates on the H 2 100 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. average from P, only one-third as much, as tlie man himself. This value of -^ is four and a half times smaller than the numerical converse of f , since A^, or f, being multiplied into ^, is equal to f . c. Parental : As a Mid-Parental deviation is equal to one-half of the two Parental deviations, it follows that the Mid-Parental Eegression must be ecjual to one-half of the sum of the two Parental Regressions. As the latter are equal to one another it follows that all three must have the same value. In other words, the average Mid-Parental Regression being ^, the average Parental Regression must be -J- also. As there was much appearance of paradox in the above strongly contrasted results, I looked carefully into the run of the figures in Table 11. They were deduced, as already said, from a MS. chart on which the stature of every Son and the transmuted Stature of every Daughter is entered opposite to that of the Mid- Parent, the transmuted Statures being reckoned to the nearest tenth of an inch, and the position of the other entries being in every respect exactly as they were recorded. Then the number of entries in each square inch were counted, and copied in the form in which they ajDpear in the Table. I found it hard at first to catch the full significance of the entries, though 1 soon discovered curious and apparently very interesting relations between them. These came out distinctly after I had '' smoothed " the entries by writing at each intersection between a horizontal line and a ver- VII.] DISCUSSION OF THE DATA OF STATURE. 101 tical one, tlie sum of the entries in the four adjacent squares. I then noticed (see Fig. 11) that lines drawn through entries of the same value formed a series of concentric and similar ellipses. Their common centre lay at the intersection of those vertical and horizontal lines which correspond to the value of 68 J inches, as read on both the top and on the side scales. Their axes were similarly inclined. The points where each successive ellipse was touched by a horizontal tangent, lay in a straight line that was inclined to the vertical in FIG. II. A Y \ 'N / '-"/P J^\ m: k ^ ^ the ratio of f, and those where the ellipses were touched by a vertical tangent, lay in a straight line inclined to the horizontal in the ratio of \. It will be obvious on studying Fig. 11 that the point where each suc- cessive horizontal line touches an ellipse is the point at which the greatest value in the line will be found. The same is true in respect to the successive vertical lines. Therefore these ratios confirm the values of the Eatios of Eegression, already obtained by a different method, namely those of f from Mid-Parent to Son, and of 102 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chip. ^- from Son to Mid-Parent. These and other re- lations were evidently a subject for matliematical analysis and verification. It seemed clear to me that they all depended on three elementary measures, sup- posing the law of Frequency of Error to be applicable throughout ; namely (l) the value of Q in the General Population, which was found to be 17 inch; (2) the value of Q in any Co-Fraternity, which was found to be 1*5 inch; (3) the Average Eegression of the Stature of the Son from that of the Mid-Parent, which was found to be f . I wrote down these values, and phrasing the problem in abstract terms, disentangled from all refer- ence to heredity, submitted it to Mr. J. T>, Hamilton Dickson, Tutor of St. Peter's College, Cambridge (see Appendix B). I asked him kindly to investigate for me the Surface of Frequency of Error that would result from these three data, and the various shapes and other particulars of its sections that were made by horizontal planes, inasmuch as they ought to form the ellipses of which I spoke. The problem may not be difficult to an accomplished mathematician, but I certainly never felt such a glow of loyalty and respect towards the sovereignty and wide sway of mathematical analysis as when his answer arrived, confirming, by purely mathematical reasoning, my vari- ous and laborious statistical conclusions with far more minuteness than I had dared to hope, because the data ran somewhat roughly, and I had to smooth them with tender caution. His calculation corrected my observed value of Mid- Parental Eegression from ^ to yf tt '> ^^^^ VII.] DISCUSSION OF THE DATA OF STATURE. 103 relation between the major and minor axis of the ellipses was changed 3 per cent. ; and their inclination to one another was changed less than 2°.^ It is obvious from this close accord of calculation with observation, that the law of Error holds through- out with sufficient precision to be of real service, and that the various results of my statistics are not casual and disconnected determinations, but strictly interdependent. I trust it will have become clear even to the most non-mathematical reader, that the law of Eegression in Stature refers primarily to Deviations, that is, to measurements made from the level of -mediocrity to the 1 The following is a more detailed comparison between tlie calculated and tlie observed results. The latter are enclosed in brackets. The letters refer to Fig. 11 : — Given — The "Probable Error" of each system of Mid- Parentages = 1-22 inch. (This was an earlier determination of its value ; as already said, the second decimal is to be considered only as approximate.) Eatio of mean filial regression = f . " Prob. Error" of each Co-Fraternity = 1*50 inch. Sections of surface of frequency parallel to XY are true ellipses. (Obs. — Apparently true ellipses.) MX : YO = 6 : 17*5, or nearly 1:3. (Obs.— 1 : 3.) Major axes to minor axes = ^ 1 '. ^^2 = 10: 5*35. (Obs.— 10 : 5-1.) Inclination of major axes to OX = 26° 36'. (Obs. 25°. ) Section of surface parallel to XZ is a true Curve of Frequency. (Obs. — Apparently so.) " Prob. Error ", the Q of that curve, = 1.07 inch. (Obs.— 1 '00, or a little more.) 104 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. crown of the liead, upwards or downwards as the case may be, and not from the ground to the crown of the head. (In the po23ulation with which I am now dealing, the level of mediocrity is 68 J inches (without shoes).) The law of Regression in respect to Stature may be phrased as follows ; namely, that the Deviation of the Sons from P are, on the average, equal to one-third of the deviation of the Parent from P, and in the same direction. Or more briefly still : — If P -1- (zt D) be the Stature of the Parent, the Stature of the offspring will on the average be P + (zb |- D). If this remarkable law of Regression had been based only on those experiments with seeds, in which I first observed it, it might weU be distrusted until otherwise confirmed. If it had been corroborated by a compara- tively small number of observations on human stature, some hesitation might be expected before its truth could be recognised in opposition to the current belief that the child tends to resemble its parents. But more can be urged than this. It is easily to be shown that we ought to expect Filial Regression, and that it ought to amount to some constant fractional part of the value of the Mid- Parental deviation. All of this will be made clear in a subsequent section, when we shall discuss the cause of the curious statistical constancy in successive generations of a large population. In the meantime, two different reasons may be given for the occurrence of Regression ; the one is connected with our notions of stability of type, and of which no more need now be . said ; the other is as follows : — The child inherits partly from his VII.] DISCUSSION OF THE DATA OF STATURE. 105 parents, partly from liis ancestry. In every population tliat intermarries freely, wlien the genealogy of any man is traced far backwards, liis ancestry will be found to consist of such varied elements that they are indistin- guishable from a sample taken at haphazard from the general Population. The Mid- Stature IVI of the remote ancestry of such a man will become identical with P ; in other words, it will be mediocre. To put the same conclusion into another form, the most probable value of the Deviation from P, of his Mid- Ancestors in any remote generation, is zero. For the moment let us confine our attention to some one generation in the remote ancestry on the one hand, and to the Mid-Parent on the other, and ignore all other generations. The combination of the zero Devia- tion of the one with the observed Deviation of the other is the combination of nothing with something. Its efi'ect resembles that of pouring a measure of water into a vessel of wine. The wine is diluted to a con- stant fraction of its alcoholic strength, whatever that strength may have been. Similarly with regard to every other generation. The Mid-Deviation in any near generation of the ancestors will have a value intermediate between that of the zero Deviation of the remote ancestry, and of the observed Deviation of the Mid-Parent. Its combination with the Mid-Parental Deviation will be as if a mixture of wine and water in some definite proportion, and not pure water, had been poured into the wine. The process throughout is one of proportionate dilutions, and the 106 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. joint effect of all of tliem is to weaken the original alcoholic strength in a constant ratio. The law of Eegression tells heavily against the full hereditary transmission of any gift. Only a few out of many children would be likely to differ from mediocrity so widely as their Mid-Parent, and still fewer would differ as widely as the more exceptional of the two Parents. The more bountifully the Parent is gifted by nature, the more rare will be his good fortune if he begets a son who is as richly endowed as himself, and still more so if he has a son who is endowed yet more largely. But the law is even-handed ; it levies an equal succession-tax on the transmission of badness as of goodness. If it discourages the extravagant hopes of a gifted parent that his children will inherit all his powers ; it no less discountenances extravagant fears that they will inherit all his weakness and disease. It must be clearly understood that there is nothing in these statements to invalidate the general doctrine that the children of a gifted pair are much more likely to be gifted than the children of a mediocre pair. They merely express the fact that the ablest of all the children of a few gifted pairs is not likely to be as gifted as the ablest of all the children of a very great many mediocre pairs. The constancy of the ratio of Eegression, whatever may be the amount of the Mid-Parental Deviation, is now seen to be a reasonable law which might have been foreseen. It is so simple in its relations that I have VII.] DISCUSSION OF THE DATA OF STATURE. 107 FIG .12. contrived more than one form of apparatus by which the probable stature of the children of known parents can be mechanically reckoned. Fig. 12 is a representation of one of them, that is worked with pulleys and weights. A, B, and C are three thin wheels with grooves round their edges. They are screwed together so as to form a single piece that turns easily on its axis. The weights M and F are attached to either end of a thread that passes over the movable pulley D. The pulley itself hangs from a thread which is wrapped two or three times round the groove of B and is then secured to the wheel. The weight SD hangs from a thread that is wrapped two or three times round the groove of A, and is then secured to the wheel. The dia- meter of A is to that of B as 2 to 3. Lastly, a thread is wrapped in the opposite direction round the wheel C, which may have any convenient diameter, and is attached to a counterpoise. M refers to the male statures, F to the female ones, S to the Sons, D to the Daughters. The scale of Female Statures differs from that of the Males, each Female height being laid down in the position which would be occupied by its male equivalent. 108 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. Tims 56 is written in the position of 60*48 inches, wliich is equal to 56x1*08. Similarly, 60 is written in the position of 64*80, which is equal to 60 x 1*08. It is obvious that raising M will cause F to fall, and vice versa, without affecting the wheel AB, and there- fore without affecting SD ; that is to say, the Parental Differences may be varied indefinitely without affecting the Stature of the children, so long as the Mid-Parental Stature is unchanged. But if the Mid-Parental Stature is changed to any specified amount, then that of SD will be changed to f of that amount. The weights M and F have to be set opposite to the heights of the mother and father on their respective scales ; then the weight SD will show the most probable heights of a Son and of a Daughter on the corresponding scales. In every one of these cases, it is the fiducial mark in the middle of each weight by which the reading is to be made. But, in addition to this, the length of the weight SD is so arranged that it is an equal chance (an even bet) that the height of each Son or each Daughter will lie within the range defined by the upper and lower edge of the weight, on their respective scales. The length of SD is 3 inches, which is twice the Q of the Co-Fraternity ; that is, 2 x 1*50 inch. d. Fraternal : In seeking for the value of Fraternal Regression, it is better to confine ourselves to the Special data given in Table 13, as they are much more trustworthy than the R.F.F. data in Table 12. By treating them in the way shown in Fig. 13, which is constructed on the same principle as Fig. 10, page 96, VII.] DISCUSSION OF THE DxiTA OF STATURE. 109 2. . 3 ' I obtained the value for Fraternal Eegression of that is to say, the unknown brother of a known man is probably only two-thirds as exceptional in Stature as he is. This is the same value as that obtained for the Regression from Mid-Parent to Son. However para- doxical the fact may seem at first, of there being such a thing as Fraternal Regression, a little reflection will show its reasonableness, which will become much clearer later on. In the meantime, we may recollect that the F\Cr A3. FRATERNAL REGRESSION R.F.F. 64 6S 6S 70 72 SPECfALS 64 €6 68 70 73 ' I ' J .1.1 7B 70 6S 66 64 - e / / 1* / 72, 70 68 €6 6^ */ / / /' 1 1 I 1 1 1 1 1 \ 1 I 1 I— unknown brother has two different tendencies, the one to resemble the known man, and the other to resemble his race. The one tendency is to deviate from P as much as his brother, and the other tendency is not to deviate at all. The result is a compromise. As the average Regression from either Parent to the Son is twice as great as that from a man to his Brother, a man is, generally speaking, only half as nearly related 110 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [cuap. to either of his Parents as he is to his Brother. In other words, the Parental kinship is only half as close as the Fraternal. We have now seen that there is Eegression from the Parent to his Son, from the Son to his Parent, and from the Brother to his Brother. As these are the only three possible lines of kinship, namely, descending, ascending, and collateral, it must be a universal rule that the un- known Kinsman, in any degree, of a known Man, is on the average more mediocre than he. Let PitD be the stature of the known man, and PdzP)' the stature of his as yet unknown kinsman, then it is safe to wager, in the absence of all other knowledge, that D^ is less than D. Squadron of Statures.— It is an axiom of statistics, as I need hardly repeat, that every large sample taken at random out of any still larger group, may be con- sidered as identical in its composition, in such inquiries as these in which we are now engaged, where minute accuracy is not desired and where highly exceptional cases are not regarded. Suppose our larger group to consist of a million, that is of 1000 x 1000 statures, and that we had divided it at random into 1000 samples each containing 1000 statures, and made Schemes of each of them. The 1000 Schemes would be practically identical, and we mioiit marshal them one behind the other in successive ranks, and thereby form a " Squad- ron," numbering 1000 statures each way, and standing VII.] DISCUSSION OF THE DATA OF STATURE. Ill upon a square base. Our Squadron may be divided either into 1000 ranks or into 1000 files. The ranks will form a series of 1000 identical Schemes, the files will form a series of 1000 rectangles, that are of the same breadth, but of dissimilar heights. (See Fig. 14.) It is easy by this illustration to give a general idea, to be developed as we proceed, of the way in which any large sample, A, of a Population gives rise to a group of Kinsmen, Z, so distant as to retain no family likeness FIG. 14 to A, but to be statistically undistinguishable from the Population generally, as regards the distribution of their statures. In this case the samples A and Z would form similar Schemes. I must suppose provisionally, for the purpose of easily arriving at an approximate theory, that tall, short, and mediocre Parents contribute equally to the next generation though this may not strictly be the case.^ 1 Oddly enough, the shortest couple on my list have the largest family, namely, sixteen children, of whom fourteen were measured. 112 NATUIIAL INHERITANCE. [chap. Tlirow A into the form of a Squadron and not of a Scheme, and let us begin by confining our attention to the men who form any two of the rectangular files of A, that we please to select. Then let us trace their connections with their respective Kinsmen in Z. As the number of the Z Kinsmen to each of the A files is considered to be the same, and as their respective Stature-Schemes are supposed to be identical with that of the general Population, it follows that the two Schemes in Z derived from the two difi'erent rectangular files in A, will be identical with one another. Every other rectangular file in A will be similarly represented by another identical Scheme in Z. Therefore the 1,000 difi'erent rectangular files in A will produce 1,000 iden- tical Schemes in Z, arranged as in Fig. 14. Though all the Schemes in Z, contain the same number of measures, each will contain many more measures than were contained in the files of A, because the same kinsmen would usually be counted many times over. Thus a man may be counted as uncle to many nephews, and as nephew to many uncles. We will therefore (though it is hardly necessary to do so) suppose each of the files in Z to have been constructed from only a sample consisting of 1,000 persons, taken at random out of the more numerous measures to which it refers. By this treatment Z becomes an exact Squadron, consisting of 1,000 elements, both in rank and in file, and it is identical with A in its constitution, though not in its attitude. The ranks of Z, which are Schemes, have been derived from the files of A, which arc rect- VII.] DISCUSSION OF THE DATA OF STATURE. 113 angles, therefore the two Squadrons must stand at right angles to one another, as in Fig. 14. The upper surface of A is curved in rank, and horizontal in file ; that of Z is curved in file and horizontal in rank. The Kinsmen in nearer degrees than Z will be re- presented by Squadrons whose forms are intermediate between A and Z. Front views of these are shown in i FiG .15. i yi I J\ ! 11 pi — vy^ Tr. T •¥2 -S-1 -1 tis 1 •f- C 1^ Fig. 15. Consequently they will be somewhat curved both in rank and in file. Also as the Kinsmen of all the members of a Population, in any degree, are them- selves a Population having similar characteristics to those of the Population of w^hich they are part, it follows that the elements of every intermediate Squadron when they are broken up and sorted afresh into ordinary Schemes, would form identical Schemes. Therefore, it is clear that a law exists that connects the curvatures in rank and in file, of any Squadron. Both of the cur- vatures are Curves of Distribution; let us call their Q values respectively r and /. Then if p be the Q of I 114 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [c'Hap. the general Population, we arrive at a general equation tliat is true for all degrees of Kinship ; namely — r^+/^=p^ (1) but r, tlie curvature in rank, is a regressed value of p, and may be written icp, iv being the value of the Eegression. Therefore the above equation may be put in the form of %D''^''^P^f (2) in which f is the Q of the Co-kinsmen in the given degree. It will be found that the intersection of the surfaces of the Squadrons by a horizontal plane, whose height is equal to P, forms in each case a line, whose general in- clination to the ranks of A increases as the Kinship becomes more remote, until it becomes a right angle in Z. The progressive change of inclination is shown in the small squares drawn at the base of Fig. 13, in which the lines are the projections of contours drawn on the upper surfaces of the Squadrons, to correspond with the multiples there stated of values of jy. It will be understood from the front views of the four different Squadrons, which form the upper part of Fig. 13, how the Mid-Statures of the Kinsmen to the Men in each of the files of A, gradually become more mediocre in the successive stages of kinship until they all reach absolute mediocrity in Z. This figure aff*ords an excellent diagramatic representation, true to scale,- of the action of the law of Eegression in Descent. I should like to have given in addition, a perspective view of the Squadrons, but fiiilcd to draw them VII.] DISCUSSION OF THE DATA OF STATURE. 115 clearly, after making many attempts. Their curvatures are so delicate and peculiar tliat tlie eye can liardly appreciate them even in a model, without turning it about in different lights and aspects. A plaster model of an intermediate form was exhibited at the Eoyal Society by Mr. J. D. H. Dickson, when my paper on Hereditary Stature was read, together with his solutions of the problems that are given in the Appendix. I also exhibited arrangements of files and ranks that were made of pasteboard. Mr. Dixon mentioned that the mathematical properties of a Surface of Frequency showed that no strictly straight line could be drawn upon it. Successive Oenerations of a People. — We are far too apt to regard common events as matters of course, that require no explanation, whereas they may be problems of much interest and of some difficulty, and still await solution. Why is it, when we compare two large groups of persons selected at random from the same race, but belonging to different generations, that they are usually found to be closely alike ? There may be some small statistical dissimilarity due to well understood differences in the general conditions of their lives, but with this I am not concerned. The present question is as to the origin of that statistical resemblance between successive generations which is due to the strict pro- cesses of heredity, and which is commonly observed in all forms of life. T 2 116 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. In eacli generation, individuals are found to be tall and short, heavy and light, strong and weak, dark and pale ; and the proportions of those who present these several characteristics in their various degrees, tend to be constant. The records of geological history afford striking evidences of this statistical similarity. Fossil remains of plants and animals may be dug out of strata at such different levels, that thousands of genera- tions must have intervened between the periods at which they lived ; yet in large samples of such fossils we may seek in vain for peculiarities that distinguish one generation from another, the different sizes, marks, and variations of every kind, occurring with equal frequency in all. If any are inclined to reply that there is no wonder in the matter, because each individual tends to leave his like behind him, and therefore each generation must, as a matter of course, resemble the one preceding, the patent fact of Eegression shows that they utterly misunderstand the case. We have now reached a stage at which it has become possible to discuss the problem with some exactness, and I will do so by giving mathematical expression to what actually took place in the Statures of that sample of our Population whose life-histories are recorded in the E.F.F. data. The Males and Females in Generation I. whose M has the value of P (viz., GSj inches), and whose Q is 17 inch, were found to group themselves as it were at random, into couples, and then to form a system of VII.] DISCUSSION OF THE DATA OF STATURE. 117 Mid-Parents. This system had of course the same IVI as the general Population, but its Q was reduced to ^^xl'7 inch, or to 1*2 inch. It was next found w^hen the Statures of the Mid-Parents, expressed in tlie form of P-h(=tD), were sorted into groups in which D w^as the same (reckoning to the nearest inch), that a Co-fraternity sprang from each group, and that its IVI had the value of P-l-(zizfD). The system in which each element is a Mid- Co-Fraternity, must have the same M as before, of 6 8|^ inches, but its Q will be again reduced, namely from 1 '2 inch to f x 1 *2 inch, or to 0'8 inch. Lastly, the individual Co-Fraternals were seen to be dispersed from their respective Mid-Co- Fraternities, with a Q equal in each case to 1*5 inch. The sum of all of the Co-Fraternals forms the Popula- tion of Generation 11. Consequently the members of Generation II. constitute a system that has an IVI of 68^ inches and a Q equal to ^ [(0-8)'-|- (1'5)'], = 17 inch. These values are identical with those in Genera- tion I. ; so the cause of their statistical similarity is tracked out. There ought to be no misunderstanding as to the character of the evidence or of the reasoning upon which this analysis is based. A small but fair sample of the Population in two successive Generations has been taken, and its conditions as regards Stature have been strictly discussed. It was found that the distribution of Stature was sufficiently Normal to justify our ignoring any shortcomings in that respect. The transmutation 118 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap of female lieigiits to tlieir male equivalents was justified by the fact that when the individual Statures of a group of females are raised in the proportion of 100 to 108, the Scheme drawn from them fairly coincides with that drawn from male Statures. Marriage selection was found to take no sufiicient notice of Stature to be worth con- sideration ; neither was the number of children in Fraternities found to be sensibly afi^ected by the Statures of their Parents. Again, it was seen to be of no consecj^uence when dealing statistically with the offspring, whether their Parents were alike in stature or not, the only datum deserving consideration being the Stature of the Mid-Parent, that is to say, the average value of (l) the Stature of the Father, and of (2) the Transmuted Stature of the Mother. I fully grant that not one of these deductions may be strictly exact, but the error introduced into the conclusions by supposing them to be correct proves not to be worth taking into account in a first approximation. Precisely the same may be said of the ulterior steps in this analysis. Every one of them is based on the properties of an ideally perfect curve, but in no case has there been need to make any sensible departure from the observed results, except in assigning a uniform value to Q in the difi"erent Co-Fraternities. Strictly speaking, that value was found to slightly rise or fall as the Mid-Stature of the Co-Fraternity rose or fell. This suggested the advisability of treating the whole inquiry on the principle of the Geometric Mean, Appendix G. I tried that principle in what seemed to be the most VII.] DISCUSSION OF THE DATA OF STATURE. . 119 hopeful case among my 18 schemes, but found the gain, if any, to be so small, that I did not care to go on with the experiment. It did not seem to deserve the additional trouble, and I was indisposed to do anything that was not really necessary, which might further confuse the reader. But had I possessed better data, I should have tried the Geometric Mean throughout. In doing so, every measure would be replaced by its logarithm, and these logarithms would be treated just as if they had been the observed values. The conclusions to which they might lead would then be re-transmuted to the numbers of which they were the logarithmic equivalents. In short, we have dealt mathematically with an ideal population which has similar characteristics to those of a real population, and have seen how closely the' behaviour of the ideal population corresponds in every stage to that of the real one. Therefore we have arrived at a closely approximate solution of the problem of statistical constancy, though numerous refinements have been neglected. Natural Selection. — This hardly falls within the scope of our incjuiry into Natural Inheritance, but it will be appropriate to consider briefly the way in which the action of Natural Selection may harmonise with that of pure heredity, and work together with it in such a manner as not to compromise the normal distribution of faculty. To do this, we must deal with the case that best represents the various possible 120 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. occurrences, namely tliat in wliicli tlie mediocre members of a population are those tliat are most nearly in harmony with their circumstances. The harmony ought to concern the aggregate of their faculties, combined on the principle adopted in Table 3, after weighting them in the order of their importance. We may deal with any faculty separately, to serve as an example, if its mediocre value happens to be that which is most preservative of life under the majority of circumstances. Such is Stature, in a rudely approximate degree, inas- much as exceptionally tall or exceptionally short persons have less chance of life than those of moderate size. It will give more definiteness to the reasoning to take a definite example, even though it be in part an imaginary one. Suppose then, that we are considering the stature of some animal that is liable to be hunted by certain beasts of prey in a particular country. So far as he is big of his kind, he would be better able than the mediocrities to crush through thick grass and foliage whenever he was scampering for his life, to jump over obstacles, and possibly to run somewhat faster than they. So far as he is small of his kind, he would be better able to run through narrow openings, to make quick turns, and to hide himself. Under the general circumstances, it would be found that animals of some particular stature had on the whole a better chance of escape than any other, and if their race is closely adapted to their circumstances in respect to stature, the most favoured stature would be identical with the M of the race. AVe already know that if we VII.] DISCUSSION OF THE DATA OF STATURE. 121 call this value P, and write each stature under the form of P + o^ (in which x includes its sign), and if the number of times with which any value P + cc occurs, compared to the number of times in which P occurs, be called y, then x and y are connected by the law of Frequency of Error. Though the impediments to flight are less unfavour- able, on the average, to the stature P than to any other, they will differ in different experiences. The course of one animal may chance to pass through denser foliage than usual, or the obstacles in his way may be higher. In that case an animal whose stature exceeded P would have an advantage over mediocrities. Conversely, the circumstances might be more favourable to a small animal. Each particular line of escape w^ould be most favour- able to some particular stature, and whatever the value of X might be, it is possible that the stature V-{-x might in some cases be more favoured than any other. But the accidents of foliage and soil in a country are characteristic and persistent, and may fairly be con- sidered as approximately of a typical kind. Therefore those that most favour the animals whose stature is P will be more frequently met with than those that favour any other stature P-l-x, and the frequency of the latter occurrence will diminish rapidly as x increases. If the number of times with which any particular value oiV-\-x is most favoured, as compared with the number of times in which P is most favoured, be called y\ we may fairly assume that y^ and x are 122 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. connected by the law of Frequency of Error. But though the system of y values and that of if values may be both subject to the law, it is not for a moment to be supposed that their Q values are necessarily the same. We have now to show how a large population of animals becomes reduced by the action of natural selection to a smaller one, in which the M value of the statures is unchanged, while the Q value is decreased. To do this we must first consider the population to have grown up entirely shielded from causes of pre- mature mortality ; call their number N. Then suppose them to be assailed by all the lethal influences that have no reference to stature. These would reduce their number to W, but by the hypothesis, the values of M and of Q would remain unaffected. Next let the influences that act selectively on stature, further reduce the numbers to S ; these being the final survivors. We have seen that : — ^=:the number of individuals who have the stature Pzh^, counting those who have the stature P, as 1. ?/' = the number of times in which Pzb^ is the most favoured stature, counting those in which P is the most favoured, as 1. Then yij — t]iQ number of times that individuals of the stature Pzba^ are selected, counting those in which individuals of the stature P are selected, as 1. As the relation between y and x, and between y' and X are severally governed by the law of Frequency of Error, it follows directly from the formula by which VII.] DISCUSSION OF THE DATA OF STATURE. 123 that law is expressed, that the relation between yy^ and X is also governed by it. The value of P of course remains the same throughout, but the Q in the system of yy' values is necessarily less than that in the system of y values. It might well be that natural selection would favour the indefinite increase of numerous separate faculties, if their improvement could be effected without detriment to the rest ; then, mediocrity in that faculty would not be the safest condition. Thus an increase of fleetness would be a clear gain to an animal liable to be hunted by beasts of prey, if no other useful faculty was thereby diminished. But a too free use of this *Mf" would show a jaunty disregard of a real difficulty. Organisms are so knit together that change in one direction involves change in many others ; these may not attract atten- tion, but they are none the less existent. Organisms are like ships of war constructed for a particular purpose in warfare, as cruisers, line of battle ships, &c., on the principle of obtaining the utmost efficiency for their special purpose. The result is a compromise between a variety of conflicting desiderata, such as cost, speed, accommodation, stability, weight of guns, thickness of armour, quick steering power, and so on. It is hardly possible in a ship of any long established type to make an improvement in any one of these respects, without a sacrifice in other directions. If the fleetuess is increased, the engines must be larger, and more space must be given up to coal, and this diminishes the remaining 124 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. accommoda,tion. Evolution may produce an altogether new type of vessel that shall be more efficient than the old one, but when a particular type of vessel has become adapted to its functions through long experience it is not possible to produce a mere variety of its type that shall have increased efficiency in some one particular without detriment to the rest. So it is with animals. Variability in Fraternities. — Human Fraternities are far too small to admit of their Q being satisfactorily measured by the direct method. We are obliged to have recourse to indirect methods, of which no less than four are available. I shall ajoply each of them to both the Special and to the E.F.F. data ; this mil give 8 separate estimates of its value, which in the meantime will be called h. The four methods are as follow : First method ; by Fraternities each containing the same nnmber of persons : — Let me begin by saying that I had already found in the large Fraternities of Sweet Peas, that the sizes of individuals of whom they con- sisted were normally distributed, and that their Q was independent of the size, or of the Stature as we may phrase it, of the Mid- Fraternity. We have also seen that the Q is practically the same in all Co-fraternities of men. Therefore it is reasonable to expect that it will also be found to be the same in all their Fraternities, though owing to their small size we cannot assure our- selves of the fact by direct evidence. We will assume this to be the case for the present ; it will be seen that the results justify the assumption. VII.] DISCUSSION OF THE DATA OF STATURE. 125 The value of the IVI of a small Fraternity may be much affected by the addition or subtraction even of a single member, it may therefore be called the apparent M, to be distinguished from the triie ^, from which its members would be found to be dispersed, if there had been many more of them. The apparent M approxi- mates towards the true M as the Fraternity increases in size, though at a much slower rate. We have now somehow to get at this true M. For distinction and for brevity let us call the apparent IV! of any small Fraternity (MF^), and that of the corresponding true IVI (MF). Then (MF) may be deduced from (MF') as follows : — We will begin by allowing ourselves for the moment to imagine the existence of an exceedingly large Frater- nity, far more numerous than is physiologically possible, and to suppose that its members vary among themselves just as widely, neither more nor less so, than in the small Fraternities of real life. The (MF^) of our large ideal Fraternity will therefore be identical with its (MF), and its Q will be the same as h. Next, take at random out of this huge ideal Fraternity a large number of small samples, each consisting of the same number, n, of brothers, and call the apparent Mid- values in the several samples, (MF^i), (MF^s)? &c. It can easily be shown that (MF^i), (MF^g)? &c., will be so distributed about the common centre of (MF), that the Prob. Deviation of any one of them from it, that is to say, the Q of their system will = 7> x ^. If n = 1 , then the Prob. Devia- tion becomes 6, as it should. If n = 2, the Prob. 126 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. Deviation is determined by tlie same problem as tbat wliich concerned tbe Q of the Mid-Parentages (page 87), where it was shown to be h x ~^. By similar reasoning, when 72 = 3, the Prob. Deviation becomes ^ x -^, and so on. When n is infinitely large, the Prob. Deviation is 0 ; that is to say, the (MF^) values do not differ at all from their common (MF). Now if we make a collection of human Fraternities, each consisting of the same number, n, of brothers, and note the differences between the (MF^) in each frater- nity and the individual brothers, we shall obtain a system of values. By drawing a Scheme from these in the usual way, we are able to find their Q ; let us call it d. We then determine h in terms of d, as follows : — The (MF^) values are distributed about their common (MF), each with the Prob. Deviation of h x ^^^, and the Statures of the individual Brothers are distributed about their respective (MF^) values, each with the Prob. Deviation d. The compound result is the same as if the statures of the individual brothers had been distributed about the common (MF), each with the Prob. Deviation h, consequently 5^ = (i^ -}- — , or lf= d^. ^ ^ n' n-1 I determined d by observation for four different values of n, having taken four groups of Fraternities, consisting respectively of 4, 5, 6, and 7 brothers, as shown in Table 14. Substituting these four observed values in turns for d in the above formula, I obtained VII.] DISCUSSION OF THE DATA OF STATURE. 127 four independent values of h, wliich are respectively I'Ol, 1-01, 1'20, and I'OS ; tlie mean of these is 1'07. Second Method ; from the mean value of Fraternal Eegression : — We may look on the Population as com- posed of a system of Fraternities. Call their respective true centres (see last paragraph) (MFi), (MFg), &c. These will be distributed about P with an as yet un- known Prob. Deviation, that we will call c. The individual members of each Fraternity will of course be distributed from their own (MF) with a Q equal to h. Then {l'7Y = c' + ¥ (l) Let P-l-(dzF„) be the stature of any individual, and let P+(dzMF,,) be that of the M of his Fraternity, then Problem 4 (page 69) shows us that : — , , , , ^ (MFJ . c' , , the most probable value oi —^ — - is ^^,2 , 2 (2) This is also the value of Fraternal Eegression, and therefore equal to -f. Substituting in (2), and replacing c by the value given by (l), we obtain 6=: 0*9 8 inch. Third Method ; by the Variability in the value of individual cases of Fraternal Eegression : — The figures in each line of Table 13 are found to have a Q equal to 1 '24 inch, and they are the results of two independent systems of variation. First, the several (MF) values (see last paragraph) are dispersed from the M of all of them with a Q that we will call v. Secondly the 128 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. individiial brotliers in each Fraternity are dispersed from their own (MF) with a Q equal to h. Hence {l'24.Y==v'-]-h\ he But it is shown Problem 5 that v = ,/,2 , — 2\ J 7 2 2 therefore (1-24)^ = 6^ + 5^. Substituting for c^ its value of (17)^ — 6^ (see last para- graph), we obtain 5 = 0*98 inch. Fourth Method ; from differences between pairs of brothers taken at random : — In the fourth method, Pairs of Brothers are taken at random, and the Differ- ences between the statures in each pair are noted ; then, under the following reservation, any one of these differences would have the Prob. value of \/ 2 x 5. The reservation is, that only as many Differences should be taken out of each Fraternity as are independent. A Fraternity of n brothers admits of '-^^^ possible pairs, and the same number of Differences ; but as no more than n—1 of these are independent, that number only of the Differences should be taken. I did not appre- ciate this necessity at first, and selected pairs of brothers on an arbitrary system, which had at all events the merit of not taking more than four sets of Differences from any one Fraternity however large it might be. It was faulty in taking three Differences instead of only two, out of a Fraternity of three brothers, and four Differences, instead of only three, from a Fraternity of VII.] DISCUSSION OF THE DATA OF STATURE. 129 four brothers, and therefore givang an increased weight to those Fraternities, but in other respects the system was hardly objectionable. The introduced error must be so slight as to make it scarcely worth while now to go over the work again. By the system adopted, I found the Prob. Difference to be 1'55, which divided by \^ 2 gives h =1'10 inch. Thus far we have dealt with the special data only. The less trustworthy R.F.F. give larger values of b in every case. An epitome of all the results appears in the following table : — Methods and data. Values of h obtained by different methods and from different data. From Special Data. From R.F.F. data.l (1) From Fraternities each containing the same number of persons 1-07 0-98 MO MO 1-38 1-31 M4 1-35 (2) From the mean value of Fraternal Regression (3) From the Variability cf Fraternal Regression (4) From Pairs of Brothers taken at random Mean. 1-06 The data used in the four methods are somewhat different. In (1) I could not deal with small Fraterni- 1 The E.F.F. results were obtained from brothers only and not from transmuted sisters, except in method (2), where the paucity of the data compelled me to include them, K 130 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [ciiAr. ties, so all were disregarded tliat contained fewer tlian four individuals. In (2) and (3) I could, not with, safety use large Fraternities. In (4) the method of selection was, as we have seen, quite indifferent. This makes the accordance of the results derived from the Special data all the more gratifying. Those from the E.F.F. data accord less well together. The E.F.F. measures are not sufficiently exact for use in these delicate calculations. Their results, being compounded of b and of their tendency to deviate from exactness, are necessarily too high, and should be discarded. I gather from all this that we may safely consider the value of h to be less than 1'06, and that allowing for some want of precision in the Special data, the very convenient value of 1*00 inch may reasonably be adopted. Trustivorthiness of the Constants. — There is difficulty in correcting the results obtained from the E.F.F. data, though we can make some estimate of their general inaccuracy as compared with the Special data. The reason of the difficulty is that the inaccuracy cannot be ascribed to an uncertainty of equal ± amount in every entry, such as might be due to a doubt of "shoes off" or ''shoes on." If it were so, the Prob. Error of a single value of the E.F.F. would be greater than that of one of the Specials, whereas it proves to be the same. It is likely that the inaccuracy is a com- pound first of the uncertainty above mentioned, whose effect would be to increase the value of the Prob. Error, VII ] DISCUSSION OF THE DATA OF STATURE. 131 and secondly of a tendency on tlie part of my corre- spondents to record medium statures when they were in doubt, whose effect would be to reduce the value of the Prob. Error, The E.F.R data in Table 12 run so irregularly that I cannot interpret them with aDy assurance. The value they give for Fraternal Eegression certainly does not exceed ^, and therefore a correction, amounting to no less than ^ of its amount, is required to bring it to a parity with that derived from the Special data (because i- + ^ x -J- = f). Hence it might be argued, that the value of Eegression from Mid-Parent to Son, which the E.F.F. data gave as f, ought to receive a similar correction. If so, it would be raised to |- -i- -| = f ; but I cannot believe this high value to be correct. My first estimate made from the E.F.F. data, was f , as already mentioned. If this be adopted, the corrected value would be f , or ^ instead of f , which might possibly pass. Curiously enough, this value of ^ for Eegression from Mid-Parent to Son, coincides with the value of f for Eegression from a single Parent to Son, which the direct observa- tions showed (see page 99), but which owing to their paucity and to the irregularity of the way in which they ran, I rejected and still reject, at least for the present. While sincerely desirous of obtaining a revised value of average Filial Eegression from entirely different and more accurate groups of data, the pro- visional value already adopted of f from Mid- Parent to Son may be accepted as being near enough for the present. It is impossible to revise one datum in the k2 132 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. E.F.F. series without revising all, as they hang together and support one another. General View of Kinship. — We are now able to deal with the distribution of statures among the Kinsmen in every near degree, of persons whose statures we know, but whose ancestral statures we either do hot know, or do not care to take into account. We are able to calcu- late Tables for every near degree of Kinship on the form of Table 11, and to reconstruct that same Table in a shape free from irregularities. We must first find the Regression, which we may call w, appropriate to the degree of Kinship in question. Then we calculate a value f for each line of a Table corresponding in form to that of Table 11, in which/* was found to be equal to 1*50 inch. We deduce the value of y from that of w by means of the general equation pV-{-f^=p^, p being equal to 1*7 inch. The values to be inserted in the several lines are then calculated from the ordinary table (Table 5) of the "probability integral." As an example of the first part of the process, let us suppose we are about to construct a table of Uncles and their Nephews, we find w and f as follows : A Nephew is the son of a Brother, therefore in this case we have t(;r=ixf=:f ; whence /= 1*66. The Regression, which we call lu, is a convenient and correct measure of family likeness. If the resemblance of the Kinsman to the Man, was on the average as perfect as that of the Man to his own Self, there would be no Regression at all, and the value of lo would be 1. VII.J DISCUSSION OF THE DxVTA OF STATURE. 133 Table of Data for calculating Tables of Distribution of Stature among the Kinsmen of Persons whose Stature is KNOWN. From group of persons of the same Stature, to their Kinsmen in various near degrees. Mean regression=w. Q = f = PXV(1-W2). Mid-parents to Sons 2/3 2/3 1/3 2/9 1/9 2/27 1-27 1-27 1-60 1-66 ^ Practically J that of Popu- ) lation, or ( 1'7 inch. Brothers to Brothers Fathers or Sons to "1 Sons or Fathers j Uncles or Nephews to ) Nephews or Uncles j Grandsons to Grandparents... Cousins to Cousins On the other hand, if the Kinsmen were on the average no more like the Man than if they had been a group picked at random out of the general Population, then the Eegression to P would be complete. The IVI of the Kinsmen, which is expressed by P + t(^(dzD), would in that case become P, whatever might have been the value of D ; therefore w must = 0. We see by the preceding Table that as a general rule. Fathers or Sons should be held to be only one-half as near in blood as Brothers, and Uncles and Nephews to be one- third as near in blood as Brothers. Cousins are 4-|^ times as remote as Fathers or as Sons, and 9 times as remote as Brothers. I do not extend the table further, because considera- tions would have to be taken into account that will be discussed in the next Section. The remarks made in a previous chapter about 134 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. heritages tliat blend and those that are mutually exclu- sive, must be here borne in mind. It would be a poor prerogative to inherit say the fifth part of the peculiarity of some gifted ancestor, but the chance of 1 to 5, of inheriting the whole of it, would be deservedly prized. Separate Contribution of each Ancestor. — In making the statement that Mid-Parents whose Stature is P±D have children whose average stature is PzhfD, it is supposed that no separate account has been taken of the previous ancestry. Yet though nothing may be known of them, something is tacitly impKed and has been tacitly allowed for, and this requires to be elimi- nated before we can learn the amount of the Parental bequest, pure and simple. What that something is, we must now try to discover. When speaking of converse Eegression, it was shown that a peculiarity in a Man implied a peculiarity of J- of that amount in his Mid- Parent. Call the peculiarity of the Mid-Parent D, then the implied peculiarity of the Mid-Parent of the Mid- Parent, that is of the Mid- Grand-Parent of the Man, would on the above supposition be J^D, that of the Mid- Great- Grand- Parent would be ^D, and so on. Hence the total bequeathable property would amount to I>(l+i + i + &c.)=Df. Do the bequests from each of the successive genera- tions reach the child without any, or what, diminution by the way ? I have not sufficient data to yield a direct reply, and must therefore try limiting suppositions. First, suppose the bequests by the various generations VII.] DISCUSSION OF THE DATA OF STATURE. 135 to be equally taxed ; then, as an accumulation of ances- tral contributions whose sum amounts to Df yields an effective heritage of only Df, it follows that each piece of heritable property must have been reduced to f of its original amount, because f x f = §. Secondly, suppose the tax not to be uniform, but to be repeated at each successive transmission, and to be equal to - of the amount of the property at each stage. In this case the effective heritage would be I^ I - + TT^ + 7^2^ ^ — ) = I) , which must, as before, be equal to Dl- : whence - = — ^ ^ r 11 Thirdly, it might possibly be supposed that the Mid- Ancestor in a remote generation should on the average contribute more to the child than the Mid-Parent, but this is quite contrary to what is observed. The descend- ants of what was " pedigree wheat," after being left to themselves for many generations, show little or no trace of the remarkable size of their Mid- Ancestors in the generations just before they were left to themselves, though the offspring of those Mid-Ancestors in the first generation did so unmistakably. The results of our only two valid limiting suppositions are therefore, (l) that the Mid-Parental peculiarities, pure and simple, influence the offspring to ^ of their amount ; (2) that they influence it to ^ of their amount. These values differ but slightly from ^, and their mean is closely -|, so we may fairly accept that result. Hence 136 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. tlie influence, pure and simple, of the Mid-Parent may be taken as ^, and that of tlie Mid- Grand- Parent as ^, and so on. Consequently the influence of the individual Parent would be ^, and of the individual Grand-Parent ^, and so on. It would, however, be hazardous on the present slender basis, to extend this sequence with con- fidence to more distant generations. Pedigree Moths. — I am endeavouring at this moment to obtain data that will enable me to go further, by breed- ing Pedigree Moths, thanks to the aid of Mr. Frederick Merrifield. The moths Selenia Illustraria and Illunaria are chosen for the purpose, partly on account of their being what is called double brooded ; that is to say, they pass normally through two generations in a single year, which is a great saving of time to the experimenter. They are hardy, prolific and variable, and are found to stand chloro- form well, previously to being measured and then paired. Every mfember of each Fraternity is preserved along three lines of descent — one race of long-winged moths, one of medium-winged, and one of short- winged moths. The three parallel sets are reared under identical con- ditions, so that the medium series supplies a trustworthy relative base, from which to measure the increasing divergency of the others. No one can be sure of the success of any extensive breeding experiment, but this attempt has been well started and seems to present no peculiar difficulty. Among other reasons for choosing moths for the purpose, is that they are born adults, not changing in stature after they have emerged from the chrysalis and shaken out their wings. Their families VII.] DISCUSSION OF THE DATA OF STATURE. 137 are of a convenient size for statistical purposes, say from 50 to 100, neither too few to make satisfactory Schemes, nor unmanageably large. They can be mounted as we all know, after their death, with great facility, and be remeasured at leisure. An intelligent and expe- rienced person can carry on a large breeding establish- ment in a small room, supplemented by a small garden. The methods used and the results up to last spring, have been described by Mr. Merrifield in papers read February and December 1887, and printed in the Tran- sactions of the Entomological Society. I speak of this now, in hopes of attracting the attention of some who are competent and willing to carry on collateral experi- ments with the same breed, or with altogether different species of moths. CHAPTER VIII. DISCUSSION OF THE DATA OF EYE COLOUR. Preliminary Remarks. — Data. — Persistence of Eye-Colour in tlie Popula- tion.— Fundamental Eye-Colours. — Principles of Calculation. — Results. Preliminary Remarhs. — In this chapter I will test the conclusions respecting stature by an examination into hereditary Eye-colour. Supposing all female measures to have been transmuted to their male equi- valents, it has been shown (l) that the possession of each unit of peculiarity of stature in a man [that is of each unit of difference from the average of his race] when the man's ancestry is unknown, implies the exist- ence on an average of just one- third of a unit of that jDCculiarity in his "Mid-Parent," and consequently of the same amount in each of his parents ; also just one- third of a unit in his Son ; (2) that each unit of pecu- liarity in each ancestor taken singly, is reduced in transmission according to the following average scale ; — a Parent transmits only |-, and a Grand-Parent only -^^. Stature and Eye-colour are not only different as qualities, but they are more contrasted in hereditary CH. VIII.] DISCUSSION OF THE DATA OF EYE COLOUR. 139 behaviour than perhaps any other common qualities. Parents of different Statures usually transmit a blended heritage to their children, but parents of different Eye- colours usually transmit an alternative heritage. If one parent is as much taller than the average of his or her sex as the other parent is shorter, the Statures of their children will be distributed, as we have already seen, in nearly the same way as if the parents had both been of medium height. But if one parent has a light Eye- colour and the other a dark Eye-colour, some of the children will, as a rule, be light and the rest dark ; they will seldom be medium eye-coloured, like the children of medium eye-coloured parents. The blending in Stature is due to its being the aggregate of the quasi- independent inheritances of many separate parts, while Eye-colour appears to be much less various in its origin. If notwithstanding this two-fold difference between the qualities of Stature and Eye-colour, the shares of hereditary contribution from the various ancestors are alike in the two cases, as I shall show that they are, we may with some confidence expect that the law by which those hereditary contributions are found to be governed, may be widely, and perhaps universally applicable. Data. — My data for hereditary Eye-colour are drawn from the same collection of "Eecords of Family Faculties" ("E.F.F.") as those upon which the inquiries into hereditary Stature were principally based. I have analysed the general value of these data in respect to 140 NATURAL INHERITANCK [chap. Stature, and sliown that they were fairly trustworthy. I think they are somewhat more accurate in respect to Eye-colour, upon which family portraits have often furnished direct information, while indirect information has been in other cases obtained from locks of hair that were preserved in the family as mementos. Persistence of Eye-colour in the Population. — The first subject of our inquiry must be into the existence of any slow change in the statistics of Eye-colour in the English j)opulation, or rather in that particular part of it to which my returns apply, that ought to be taken into account before drawing hereditary conclusions. For this purpose I sorted the data, not according to the year of birth, but according to generations, as that method best accorded with the particular form in which all my E.F.F. data are compiled. Those persons who ranked in the Family Kecords as the '^ children '^ of the pedigree, were counted as generation I. ; their parents, uncles and aunts, as generation 11. ; their grandparents, great uncles, and great aunts, as generation III. ; their great grandparents, and so forth, as generation IV. No account was taken of the year of birth of the " children," except to learn their age ; consequently there is much overlapping of dates in successive generations. We may however safely say, that the persons in generation I. belong to quite a different period to those in genera- tion III., and the persons in II. to those in IV. I had intended to exclude all children under the age of eight years, but in this particular branch of the inquiry, I viil] discussion OF THE DATA OF EYE COLOUR. 141 fear tliat some cases of young cliildren have been acci- dentally included. I would willingly have taken a later limit than eight years, but could not spare the data that would in that case have been lost to me. A great variety of terms are used by the various compilers of the " Family Kecords " to express Eye- colours. I began by classifying them under the follow- ing eight heads ; — 1, light blue ; 2, blue, dark blue ; 3, grey, blue-green ; 4, dark grey, hazel ; 5, light brown ; 6, brown ; 7, dark brown ; 8, black. Then I constructed Table 15. • The diagram, page 143, clearly conveys the signifi- cance of the figures in Table 15. Considering that the groups into which the observations are divided are eiffht in number, the observations are far from beino; sufiiciently numerous to justify us in expecting clean results ; nevertheless the curves come out surprisingly well, and in accordance with one another. There can be little doubt that the change, if any, during four successive generations is very small, and much smaller than mere memory is competent to take note o£ I therefore disregard a current popular belief in the exist- ence of a gradual darkening of the British population, and shall treat the eye-colours of those classes of our race who have contributed the records, as having been statistically persistent during the period under discussion. The concurrence of the four curves for the four several generations, affords internal evidence of the trustworthiness of the data. For supposing we had 142 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. curves that exactly represented the true Eye- colours for the four generations, they would either be concurrent or they would not. If these curves were concurrent, the errors in the R.F.F. data must have been so curiously distributed as to preserve the concurrence. If these curves were not concurrent, then the errors in the E.F.F. data must have been so curiously distri- buted as to neutralise the non- concurrence. Both of these suppositions are improbable, and we must con- clude that the curves really agree, and that the R.F.F. errors are not large enough to spoil the agreement. The close similarity of the two curves, derived respec- tively from the whole of the male and the whole of the female data, and the more perfect form of the curve derived from the aggregate of all the cases, are additional evidences in favour of the goodness of the data on the whole. Fundamental Eye-colours. — It is agreed among writers {cf. A. de Candolle, see footnote overleaf) that the one important division of eye-colours is into the light and the dark. The medium tints are not numerous, but may be derived from any one of four distinct origins. They may be hereditary with no notable variation, they may be varieties of light parentage, they may be varieties of dark parentage, or they may be blends. Medium tints are classed in my list under the heading "4. Dark grey, hazel;" these form only 127 per cent, of all the observed cases. In medium tints, the outer portion of the iris is often of a dark grey colour, mi.] DISCUSSION OF THE DATA OF EYE COLOUR. 143 Percentages of the Various Eye-colours in Four Successive Generations. Number of cases ,Q Sb .^ a 0 be g .0 'd ^ -a S aT ^ •¥ 1l % ^ 2 ^ to M O H 913 1515 1477 5S5 2277 2213 Generation IV, Total Males ,, Females 4490 Total cases IG .16. 144 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [cuap. and the inner of a hazel. The proportion between the grey and the hazel varies in different cases, and the eye-colour is then described as dark grey or as hazel, according to the colour that happens most to arrest the attention of the observer. For brevity, I will henceforth call all intermediate tints by the one name of hazel. I will now investigate the history of those hazel eyes that are variations from light or from dark respectively, or that are blends between them. It is reasonable to suppose that the residue which were inherited from hazel-eyed parents, arose in them or in their prede- cessors either as variations or as blends, and therefore the result of the investigation will enable us to assort the small but troublesome group of hazel eyes in an equitable proportion between light and dark, and thus to simplify our inquiry. The family records include 168 families of brothers and sisters, counting only those who were above eight years of age, in whom one member at least had hazel eyes. For distinction I will describe these as " hazel- eyed families ; " not meaning thereby that all the children have that peculiarity, but only one or more of them. The total number of the brothers and sisters in the 168 hazel-eyed families is 948, of whom 302 or about one-third have hazel eyes. The eye-colours of all the 2 x 168, or 336 parents, are given in the records, but only those of 449 of the grandparents, whose number would be 672, were it not for a few cases of cousin marriages. Thus I have information concerning VIII.] DISCUSSION OF THE DATA OF EYE COLOUR. 145 about only two-tliirds of the grandparents, but this will suffice for our purpose. The results are given in Table 16. It will be observed that the distribution of eye-colour among the grandparents of the hazel-eyed families is nearly identical with that among the population at large. But among the parents there is a notable difference ; they have a decidedly larger percentage of light eye-colour and a slightly smaller proportion of dark, while the hazel element is nearly doubled. A similar change is superadded in the children. The total result in passing from generations III. to I., is that the percentage of the light eyes is diminished from 60 or 61 to 45, therefore by one quarter of its original amount, and that the percentage of the dark eyes is diminished from 26 or 27 to 23, that is by about one- eighth of its original amount, the hazel element in either case absorbiDg the difference. It follows that the chance of a light-eyed parent having hazel off- spring, is about twice as great as that of a dark-eyed parent. Consequently, since hazel is twice as likely to be met with in any given light-eyed family as in a given dark- eyed one, we may look upon two-thirds of the hazel eyes as being fundamentally light, and one- third of them as fundamentally dark. I shall allot them rateably in that proportion between light and dark, as nearly as may be without using fractions, and so get rid of them. M. Alphonse de CandoUe^ has 1 Heredite de la Conleur des Yeux dans I'Espece humaine," par M. Alphonse de Candolle. " Arch. Sc. Phys. et Nat. Geneva," Aug. 1884, 3rd period, vol. xii. p. 97. 146 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. also shown from liis data, that yeux gris (which I take to be the equivalent of my hazel) are referable to a light ancestry rather than to a dark one, but his data are numerically insufficient to warrant a precise estimate of the relative frequency of their derivation from each of these two sources. In the following discussion I shall deal only with those fraternities in which the Eye-colours are known of the two Parents and of the four Grand- Parents. There are altogether 211 of such groups, containing an aggregate of 1023 children. They do not, however, belong to 211 different family stocks, because each stock which is complete up to the great grand-parents inclusive (and I have fourteen of these) is capable of yielding three such groups. Thus, group 1 contains a, the ^' children ; " h, the parents ; c, the grand- parents. Group 2 contains a, the father of the " children " and his brothers and his sisters ; h, the parents of the father ; c, the grand-parents of the father. Group 3 contains the corresponding selections on the mother's side. Other family stocks furnish two groups. Gufc of these and other data. Tables 19 and 20 have been made. In Table 19 I have grouped the families together whose two parents and four grand- parents present the same combination of Eye-colour, no group, however, being accepted that contains less than twenty children. The data in this table enable us to test the average correctness of the law I desire to verify, because many persons and many families appear in the same grouj), and individual peculiarities viil] discussion OF THE DATA OF EYE COLOUR. 147 tend to neutralise each other. In Table 20 I have separately classified on the same system all the families, 78 in number, that consist of six or more children. These data enable us to test the trustworthiness of the law as applied to individual families. It will be seen from my way of discussing them, that smaller fraternities than these could not be advantageously dealt with. It will be noticed that I have not printed the number of dark- eyed children in either of these tables. They are implicitly given, and are instantly to be found by subtracting the number of light-eyed children from the total number of children. Nothing would have been gained by their insertion, while compactness would have been sacrificed. The entries in the tables are classified, as I said, according to the various combinations of light, hazel, and dark Eye-colours in the Parents and Grand-Parents. There are six different possible combinations among the two Parents, and 15 among the four Grand-Parents, making 6 x 15, or 90 possible combinations altogether. The number of observations are of course by no means evenly distributed among the classes. I have no returns at all under more than half of them, while the entries of two light-eyed Parents and four light-eyed Grand- Parents are proportionately very numerous. The question of marriage selection in respect to Eye-colour, has been already discussed briefly in p. 86. It is a less simple statistical question than at a first sight it may appear to be. so I will not discuss it farther. L 2 148 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. Principles of Calculation. — I have next to show how the expectation of Eye-colour among the children of a given family is to be reckoned on the basis of the same law that held in respect to stature, so that calculations of the probable distribution of Eye-colours may be made. They are those that fill the three last columns of Tables 19 and 20, which are headed I., IL, and III., and are placed in juxtaposition with the observed facts entered in the column hea.ded " Observed." These three columns contain calculations based on data limited in three difi'erent ways, in order the more thoroughly to test the applicability of the law that it is desired to verify. Column I. contains calculations based on a knowledge of the Eye-colours of the Parents only ; II. contains those based on a knowledge of those of the Grand-Parents only ; III. contains those based on a knowledge of those both of the Parents and of the Grand-Parents, and of them only. I. Eye-colours given of the two Parents — Let the letter S be used as a symbol to signify the subject (or person) for whom the expected heritage is to be calculated. Let F stand for the words " a parent of S ; " Gi for " a grandparent of S ; " Go for "a great- grandparent of S," and so on. We must begin by stating the problem as it would stand if Stature was under consideration, and then modify it so as to apply to Eye-colour. Suppose then, that the amount of the peculiarity of Stature j)os- sesscd by F is equal to D, and that nothing whatever VIII.] DISCUSSION OF THE DATA OF EYE COLOUR. 149 is known with certainty of any of tlie ancestors of S except F. We have seen that though nothing may actually be known, yet that something definite is implied about the ancestors of F, namely, that each of his two parents (who will stand in the order of relationship of Gi to S) will on the average possess J^D. Similarly that each of the four grandparents of F (who will stand in the order of G2 to S) will on the average possess ^D, and so on. Again we have seen that F, on the average, transmits to S only J of his peculiarity ,- that Gi transmits only ^^ ; G2 only^^^, and so on. Hence the aggregate of the heritages that may be expected to converge through F upon S, is contained in the following series : — That is to say, each parent must in this case be considered as contributino; 0"30 to the heritasre of the child, or the two parents together as contributing 0*60, leaving an indeterminate residue of 0*40 due to the influence of ancestry about whom nothing is either known or implied, except that they may be taken as members of the same race as S. In applying this problem to Eye-colour, we must bear in mind that the fractional chance that each member of a family will inherit either a light or a dark Eye- colour, must be taken to mean that that same fraction 150 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. of tlie total number of cliilclren in the family will probably possess it. Also, as a consequence of this view of the meaning of a fractional chance, it follows that the residue of 0*40 must be rateably assigned between light and dark Eye-colour, in the proportion in which those Eye-colours are found in the race generally, and this was seen to be (see Table 16) as 61*2 :26'1 ; so I allot 0*28 out of the above residue of 0'40 to the heritage of light, and 0"12 to the heritage of dark. When the parent is hazel-eyed I allot f of his total contribution of 0'30, i.e., 0*20 to light, and ■J-, i.e. O'lO to dark. These chances are entered in the first pair of columns headed I. in Table 17. The pair of columns headed I. in Table 18 shows the way of summing the chances that are given in the columns that have a similar heading in Table 17. By the method there shown, I calculated all the entries that appear in the columns with the heading I. in Tables 19 and 20. II. Eye-colours given of the four Grand Parents — Suppose D to be possessed by Gi and that nothing whatever is known with certainty of any other ancestor of S. Then it has been shown that the child of Gj (that is F) will possess ^D ; that each of the two parents of Gi (who stand in the relation of G2 to S) will also possess -|-D ; that each of the four grandparents of Gi (who stand in the relation of G3 to S) will possess -^D, and so on. Also it has been shown that the shares of their several peculiarities that will on the average be transmitted by F, Gj, Go, &c., are \, y\j-, ^i^, &c., VIII.] DISCUSSION OF THE DATA OF EYE COLOUR. 151 respectively. Hence tlie aggregate of the probable heritages from Gi are expressed by the following series : — d/1x -+1 xi+lx 2 x3^-+-x 4 xi+&c. ] il2 V2* 3x2^ 3^x26 Jl Vl2 40 So that each grandparent contributes on the average 0*16 (more exactly 0'1583) of his peculiarity to the heritage of S, and the four grandparents contribute between them 0*64, leaving 36 indeterminate, which when rateably assigned gives 0"25 to light and 0*11 to dark. A hazel-eyed grandparent contributes, accord- ing to the ratio described in the last paragraph, 0*10 to light and 0*06 to dark. All this is clearly expressed and employed in the columns IL of Tables 17 and 18. III. Eye- colours given of the two Parents and four Grand-Parents — Suppose F to possess D, then F taken alone., and not in connection with what his possession of D might imply concerning the contributions of the previous ancestry, will contribute an average of 0*25 to the heritage of S. Suppose Gi also to possess D, then his contribution together with what his possession of D may imply concerning the previous ancestry, was calculated in the last paragraph as D x ^^^ =D x 0'075. For the con- venience of using round numbers I take this as DxO"08. So the two parents contribute between 152 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. them 0'50 of the peculiarity of S, the four grand- parents together with what they imply of the previous ancestry contribute 0*32, being an aggregate of 0*82, leaving a residue of 0"18 to be rateably assigned as 0'12 to light, and 0'6 to dark. A hazel-eyed Parent is here reckoned as contributing 0'16 to light and 0*9 to dark; a hazel-eyed Grand-Parent as contributing 0*5 to light and 0*3 to dark. All this is tabulated in Table 17, and its working exjDlained by an example in the columns headed III. of Table 1 8. Results. — A mere glance at Tables 19 and 20 will show how surprisingly accurate the predictions are, and therefore how true the basis of the calculations must be. Their average correctness is shown best by the totals in Table 19, which give an aggregate of calculated numbers of light-eyed children under Groups I., II., and III. as 623, 601, and 614 respectively, when the observed numbers were 629 ; that is to say, they are correct in the ratios of 99, 96, and 98 to 100. Their trustworthiness when applied to individual families is shown as strongly in Table 20 whose results are conveniently summarised in Table 21. I have there classified the amounts of error in the several calculations : thus if the estimate in any one family was 3 light- eyed children, and the observed number was 4, I should count the error as 1 '0. I have worked to one place of decimals in this table, in order to bring out the different shades of trustworthiness in the three sets of calcula- tions, which thus become very apparent. It will be VIII.] DISCUSSION OF THE DATA OF EYE COLOUR. 153 seen that the calculations in Class III. are by far the most precise. In more than one-half of those calcula- tions the error does not exceed 0 "5, whereas in more than three -quarters of those in I. and II. the error is at least of that amount. Only one-quarter of Class III., but some- where about the half of Classes I. and II., are more than 1*1 in error. In comparing I. with 11. , we find I. to be slightly but I think distinctly the superior estimate. The relative accuracy of III. as compared with I. and II., is what we should have expected, supposing the basis of the calculations to be true, because the addi- tional knowledge utilised in III., over what is turned to account in I. and II., must be an advantage. My returns are insufficiently numerous and too subject to uncertainty of observation, to make it worth while to submit them to a moi*e rigorous analysis, but the broad conclusion to which the present results irresistibly lead, is that the same peculiar hereditary relation that was shown to subsist between a man and each of his ancestors in respect to the quality of Stature, also subsists in respect to that of Eye-colour. CHAPTER IX THE ARTISTIC FACULTY. Data. — Sexual Distribution. — Marriage Selection. — Regression. — Effect of Bias in Marriage. Data. — It is many years since I described the family history of the great Painters and Musicians in Here- ditary Genius. The inheritance of much less excep- tional gifts of Artistic Faculty will be discussed in this chapter, and from an entirely different class of data. They are the answers in my R.F.F collection, to the question of ^' Favourite pursuits and interests ? Artistic aptitudes ? " The list of persons who were signalised as being especially fond of music and drawing, no doubt includes many who are artistic in a very moderate degree. Still they form a fairly well defined class, and one that is easy to discuss because their family history is complete. In this respect, they are much more suitable subjects for statistical inquiry than the great Painters and Musicians, whose biographers usually say little or nothing of their non-artistic relatives. CHAP. IX.] THE ARTISTIC FACULTY. 155 The object of the present chapter is not to give a reply to the simple question, whether or no the Artistic faculty tends to be inherited. A man must be very crotchety or very ignorant, who nowadays seriously doubts the inheritance either of this or of any other faculty. The question is whether or no its inheritance follows a similar law to that which has been shown to govern Stature and Eye- colour, and which has been worked out with some completeness in the foregoing chapters. Before answering this question, it will be convenient to compare the distribution of the Artistic faculty in the two sexes, and to learn the influence it may exercise on marriage selection. I begcin by dividing my data into four classes of aptitudes ; the first was for music alone ; the second for drawing alone ; the third for both music and drawing ; and the fourth includes all those about whose artistic capacities a discreet silence was observed. After prefatory trials, I found it so difficult to separate aptitude for music from aptitude for drawing, that I determined to throw the three first classes into the single group of Artistic. This and the group of the Non- Artistic are the only two divisions now to be considered. A difficulty presented itself at the outset in respect to the families that included boys, girls, and young children, whose artistic tastes and capacities can seldom be fairly judged, while they are liable to be appraised too favourably by the compiler of the Family records, especially if he or she was one of their parents. As the practice of picking and choosing is very hazardous in 156 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. statistical inquiries, however fair our intentions may be, and as it in justice always excites suspicion, I decided, though with much regret at their loss, to omit the wdiole of those who were not adult. Sexual Distribution. — Men and women, as classes, may differ little in their natural artistic capacity, but such difference as there is in adult life is some- what in favour of the women. Table 9b contains 894 cases, 447 of men and 447 of women, divided into three groups according to the rank they hold in the pedigrees. These groups agree fairly well among themselves, and therefore their aggregate results may be freely accepted as trustworthy. They sho^v that 28 per cent, of the males are Artistic and 72 are Not Artistic, and that there are 33 per cent. Artistic females to 67 who are Not Artistic. Part of this female superiority is doubtless to be ascribed to the large share that music and drawing occupy in the education of w^omen, and to the greater leisure that most girls have, or take, for amusing themselves. If the artistic gifts of men and women are naturally the same, as the experience of schools where music and drawing are taught, apparently shows it to be, the small difference observed in favour of women in adult life would be a measure of the smallness of the effect of education compared to that of natural talent. Dis- regarding the distinction of sex, the figures in Table 9b show that the number of Artistic to Non-Artistic persons in the general population is in the proportion IX.] THE ARTISTIC FACULTY. 157 of 30| to 69^. The data used in Table 22 refer to a considerably larger number of persons, and do not include more tban two-thirds of those employed in Table 9b, and they make the proportion to be 31 to 69. So we shall be quite correct enough if we reckon that out of ten persons in the families of my E.F.F. correspondents, three on the average are artistic and seven are not. Marriage Selection. — Table 9 b enables us to ascer- tain whether there is any tendency, or any disinclination among the Artistic and the Non- Artistic, to marry within their respective castes. It shows the observed fre- quency of their marriages in each of the three possible combinations ; namely, both husband and wife artistic ; one artistic and one not ; and both not artistic. The Table also gives the calculated frequency of the three classes, supposing the pairings to be regulated by the laws of chance. There is I think trustworthy evidence of the existence of some slight disinclination to marry within the same caste, for signs of it appear in each of the three sets of families with which the Table deals. The total result is that there are only 36 per cent, of such marriages observed, whereas if there had been no disinclination but perfect indifference, the number would have been raised to 42. The difference is small and the figures are few, but for the above reasons it is not likely to be fallacious. I believe the facts to be, that highly artistic people keep pretty much to themselves, but that the very much larger body of 158 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. moderately artistic people do not. A man of liiglily artistic temperament must look on those who are deficient in it, as barbarians ; he would continually crave for a sj^mpathy and response that such persons are incapable of giving. On the other hand, every quiet unmusical man must shrink a little from the idea of wedding himself to a grand piano in constant action, with its vocal and peculiar social accompaniments ; but he might anticipate great pleasure in having a wife of a moderately artistic temperament, who would give colour and variety to his prosaic life. On the other hand, a sensitive and imaginative wife would be con- scious of needing the aid of a husband who had enough plain common- sense to restrain her too enthusiastic and frequently foolish projects. If wife is read for husband, and husband for wife, the same argument still holds true. Eegression. — Having disposed of these preliminaries, we will now examine into the conditions of the inherit- ance of the Artistic Faculty. The data that bear upon it are summarised in Table 22, where I have not cared to separate the sexes, as my data are not immerous enough to allow of more subdivision than can be helped. Also, because from such calculations as I have made, the hereditary influences of the two sexes in respect to art appear to be pretty equal : as they are in respect to nearly every other characteristic, exclu- sive of diseases, that I have examined. It is perfectly conceivable that the Artistic Faculty IX.] THE ARTISTIC FACULTY. 159 in any person might be somehow measured, and its amount determined, just as we may measure Strength, the power of Discrimination of Tints, or the tenacity of Memory. Let us then suppose the measurement of the Artistic Faculty to be feasible and to have been often performed, and that the measures of a large number of persons were thrown into a Scheme. It is reasonable to expect that the Scheme of the Artistic Faculty would be approximately Normal in its proportions, like those of the various Qualities and Faculties whose measures were given in Tables 2 and 3. It is also reasonable to expect that the same law of inheritance might hold good in the Artistic Faculty that was found to hold good both in Stature and in Eye colour ; in other words, that the value of Filial Kegression would in this case also be f. We have now to discover whether these assumptions are true without any help from direct measurement. The problem to be solved is a pretty one, and will illustrate the method by which many problems of a similar class have to be worked. Let the graduations of the scale by which the Artistic Faculty is supposed to be measured, be such that the unit of the scale shall be equal to the Q of the Art- Scheme of the general population. Call the unknown M of the Art-Scheme of the population, P. Then, as explained in page 52, the measure of any individual will be of the form P -f (zb D), where D is the deviation from P. The first fact we have to deal with is, that only 30 per cent, of the population IGO NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. are Artistic. Therefore no person whose Grade in the Art-Scheme does not exceed 70° can be reckoned as Artistic. Referring to Table 8 we see that the value of D for the Grade of 70° is + 0*78 ; consequently the art-measure of an Artistic person, when reckoned in units of the accepted scale, must exceed P-}-0*78. The average art-measure of all persons whose Grade is higher than 70°, may be obtained with sufficient ajDproximation by taking the average of all the values given in Table 8, for every Grade, or more simply for every odd Grade from 71° to 99° inclusive. It will be found to be l*7l. Therefore an artistic person has, on the average, an art-measure of P -h l'7l. We will consider persons of this measure to be representatives of the whole of the artistic por- tion of the Population. It is not strictly correct to do so, but for approximative purposes this rough and ready method will suffice, instead of the tedious process of making a separate calculation for each Grade. The IVI of the Co -Fraternity born of a group of Mid-Parents whose measure is P -h 171 will be P -1- (f X 1-71) or (P + 1-4). We will call this value C. The Q of this or any other Co-Fraternity may be expected to bear approximately the same ratio to the Q of the general population, that it did in the case of Stature, namely, that of 1'5 to 17. Therefore the Q of the Co-Fraternity who are born of Mid-Parents whose Art-measure is C, will be 0'88. The artistic members of this Co-Fraternity will be those whose measures exceed (P -h 078}. We may write this IX.] THE ARTISTIC FACULTY. 161 value in the form of {(P + 1-4) -0-36}, or {C — 0-36}. Table 8 shows that the Deviation of— 0'36 is found at the Grade of 40°. Consequently 40 per cent, of this Co-Fraternity will be Non- Artistic and 60 per cent, will be Artistic. Observation Table 23 shows the numbers to be 36 and 64, wdiich is a very happy agreement. Next as regards the Non-Artistic Parents. The Non- Artistic portion of the Population occupy the 70 first Grades in the Art-Scheme, and may be divided into two groups ; one consisting of 40 Grades, and standing between the Grades of 70° and 30°, or between the Grade of 50° and 20 Grades on either side of it, the average Art-measure of whom is P ; the other group standing below 30°, whose average measure maybe taken to be P — 1*71, for the same reason that the group above 70° was taken as P + 1'71. Consequently the average measure of the entire Non-Artistic class is -J^{(40 X P) -h 30 (P - 1-71)} =3 P - f g X 1-71 = P - 073. Supposing Mid-Parents of this measure, to represent the entire Non- Artistic group, their offspring will be a Co- Fraternity having for their IVI the value of P — {f x 0*73} or P — 0*49, which we will call C, and for their Q the value of 0'88 as before. Such among them as exceed {P — 0'78}, which we may write in the form of {(P — 0*49) + (1'27)}, or {C + 1*27}, are Artistic, and they are those who, according to Table 8, rank higher than the Grade 83°. In other words, 83 per cent, of the children of Non- M 162 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. Artistic parents will be Non-Artistic, and tlie re- mainder of 17 per cent, will be Artistic. Observation gives the values of 79 and 21, wbicb is a very fair coincidence. When one parent is Artistic and the other Not, their joint hereditary influence would be the average of the above two cases; that is to say, -^ (40 4- 83), or 61-^ per cent, of their children would be Non -Artistic, and ^ (60 ■+ 17), or 381 would be Artistic. The observed numbers are 61 and 39, which agree excellently well. We may therefore conclude that the same law of Eegression, and all that depends upon it, which governs the inheritance both of Stature and Eye-colour, applies equally to the Artistic Faculty. Effect of Bias in Marriage. — The slight apparent disinclination of the Artistic and the Non- Artistic to marry in their own caste, is hardly worth regarding, but it is right to clearly understand the extreme effect that might be occasioned by Bias in Marriage. Suppose the attraction of like to like to become paramount, so that each individual in a Scheme married his or her nearest available neighbour, then the Scheme of Mid- Parents would be practically identical wi th the Scheme drawn from the individual members of the population. In the case of Stature their Q would be 1'7 inch, instead of 1'7 divided by \/2. The regression and subsequcut dispersion remaining unchanged, the Q of the offspring would consequently be increased. On the other hand, suppose the attraction of contrast IX.] THE ARTISTIC FACULTY. 163 to become suddenly paramount, so that Grade 99° paired in an instant witli Grade 1°; next 98° with 2°; and so on in order, until the languid desires of 49° and 51° were satisfied last of all. Then every one of the Mid-Parents would be of precisely the same stature P. Consequently their Q would be zero ; and that of the system of the Mid-Co-Fraternities would be zero also ; hence the Q of the next generation would con- tract to the Q of a Co-Fraternity, that is to 1'5 inch. Whatever might be the character or strength of the bias in marriage selection, so long as it remains constant the Q of the population would tend to become con- stant also, and the statistical resemblance between successive generations of the future Population would be ensured. The stability of the balance between the opposed tendencies of Eegression and of Co-Fraternal expansion is due to the Eegression increasing with the Deviation. Its efi'ect is like that of a spring acting against a weight ; the spring stretches until its gradually increasing resilient force balances the steady pull of the weight, then the two forces of spring and weight are in stable equilibrium. For, if the weight be lifted by the hand, it will obviously fall down again as soon as the hand is withdrawn ; or again, if it be depressed by the hand, the resilience of the spring will become increased, and the weight will rise up again when it is left free to do so. M 2 CHAPTEE X. 13ISEASE. Preliminary Problem. — Data. — Trustwortliiiiess of R.F.F. Data. — Mixture of Inheritance?. — Consumption : General Remai-ks ; Parent to Child ; Distribution of Praternities ; Severely Tainted Fraternities ; Con- snmptivity. — Data for Hereditary Diseases. The vital statistics of a population are tliose of a vast army marching rank behind rank, across the treacherous table-lancl of life. Some of its members drop out of sight at every step, and a new rank is ever rising up to take the place vacated by the rank that preceded it, and which has already moved on. The popu- lation retains its peculiarities although the elements of which it is composed are never stationary, neither are the same individuals present at any two successive epochs. In these respects, a population may be com- pared to a cloud that seems to rejDOse in calm upon a mountain plateau, while a gale of wind is blowing over it. The outline of the cloud remains unchanged, although its elements are in violent movement and in a condition of perpetual destruction and renewal. Tlie X.] DISEASE. 1G5 well understood cause of sucli clouds is the deflection of a wind laden with invisible vapour, by means of the sloping flanks of the mountain, up to a level at which the atmosphere is much colder and rarer than below. Part of the invisible vapour with wdiich the wdnd was charged, becomes thereby condensed into the minute particles of water of which clouds are formed. After a while the process is reversed. The particles of cloud having been carried by the wind across the plateau, are swept down the other side of it again to a lower level, and during their descent they return into invisible vapour. Both in the cloud and in the population, there is on the one hand a continual supply and inrush of new individuals from the unseen ; they remain a while as visible objects, and then disappear. The cloud and the population are composed of elements that resemble each other in the brevity of their exist- ence, while the general features of the cloud and of the population are alike in that they abide. Preliminary Problem. — The proportion of the population that dies at each age, is well known, and the diseases of which they die are also w^ell known, but the statistics of hereditary disease are as yet for the most part contradictory and untrustworthy. It is most desirable as a preliminary to more minute inquiries, that the causes of death of a large number of persons should be traced during two successive genera- tions in somewhat the same broad way that Stature and several other peculiarities were traced in the pre- 166 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. ceding chapters. There are a certain number of recog- nized groups of disease, which we may call A, B, C, &c., and the proportion of persons who die of these diseases in each of the two generations is the same. The pre- liminary question to be determined is whether and to what extent those who die of A in the second genera- tion, are more or less often descended from those who died of A in the first generation, than would have been the case if disease were neither hereditarily transmitted nor clung to the same families for any other reason. Similarly as regards B, C, D, and the rest. This inquiry would be more difficult than those hitherto attempted, because longevity and fertility are both affected by the state of health, and the circum- stances of home life and occupation have a great effect in causing and in checking disease. Also because the father and mother are found in some notable cases to contribute disease in very different degrees to their male and female descendants. I had hoped even to the last moment, that my collection of Family Eecords would have contributed in some small degree towards answering this question, but after many attempts I find them too fragmentary for the purpose. It was a necessary condition of success to have the completed life-histories of many Fraternities who were born some seventy or more years ago, that is, during the earlier part of this century, as well as those of their parents and all their uncles and aunts. My Records contain excellent material of a later date, that will be vahiable in future years ; but they must X.] DISEASE. 167 bide tlieir time ; they are insufficient for the period in question. By attempting to work with incompleted life histories the risk of serious error is incurred. Data, — The Schedule in Appendix G, which is illus- trated in more detail by Tables A and B that follow it, shows the amount of information that I had hoped to obtain from those' who were in a -position to furnish complete returns. It relates to the "Subject" of the pedigree and to each of his 1 4 direct ancestors, up to the great-grandparents inclusive, making in all 15 persons. Also, to the Fraternities of which each of these 15 per- sons was a member. Eeckoning the total average number of persons in each fraternity at 5, which is under the mark for my R.F.F collection, questions were thus asked concerning an average of 75 different persons in each family. The total number of the Eecords that I am able to use, is about 160; so the aggregate of the returns of disease ought to have been about twelve thousand, and should have included the causes of death of perhaps 6,000 of them. As a matter of fact, I have only about one-third of the latter number, Trustivorthiness of R.F.F. data. — The first object was to ascertain the trustworthiness of the medical informa- tion sent to me. There is usually much disinclination among families to allude to the serious diseases that they fear to inherit, and it was necessary to learn whether this tendency towards suppression notably vitiated the returns. The test applied was both simple and just. 168 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. If consumption, cancer, drink and suicide, appear among tlie recorded cases of death less frequently than they do in ordinary tables of mortality, then a bias towards suppression could be proved and measured, and would have to be reckoned wnth ; otherwise the returns might be accepted as being on the whole honest and outspoken. T find the latter to be the case. Sixteen per cent, of the causes of death (or 1 in 6^) are ascribed to consump- tion, 5 per cent, to cancer, and nearly 2 per cent. to drink and to suicide respectively. Insanity was not specially asked about, as I did not think it wise to put too many disagreeable questions, however it is often mentioned. I dare say that it, or at least eccentricity, is not unfrequently passed over. Careful accuracy in framing the replies appears to have been the rule rather than the exception. In the preface to the blank forms of the Records of Family Faculties .and elsewhere, I had explained my objects so fully and they were so reason- able in themselves, that my correspondents have evidently entered with interest into what was asked for, and shown themselves willing to trust me freely with their family histories. They seem generally to have given all that was known to them, after making much search and many inquiries, and after due references to registers of deaths. The insufiiciency of their returns proceeds I feel sure, much less from a desire to suppress unpleasant truths than from pure ignorance, and the latter is in no small part due to the scientific ineptitude of the mass of the members of the medical profession two and more generations ago, when even the stetho- X.] DISEASE. 109 scope was unknown. They were then incompetent to name diseases correctly. Mixture of Inheritances. — The first tiling that struck me after methodically classifying the diseases of each family, in the form shown in the Schedule, w^as their great intermixture. The Tables A and B in Appendix Gl- are offered as ordinary specimens of what is everywhere to be found. They are actual cases, except that I have given fancy names and initials, and for further conceal- ment, have partially transposed the sexes. Imagine an intermarriage between any two in the lower division of these tables, and then consider the variety of inheritable disease to wdiicli their children would be liable ! The problem is rendered yet more complicated by the metamorphoses of disease. The disease A in the parent does not necessarily appear, even when inherited, as A in the children. We know very little indeed about the effect of a mixture of inheritable diseases, how far they are mutually exclusive and how far they blend ; or how far when they blend, they change into a third form. Owing to the habit of free inter-marriage i)o person can be exempt from the inheritance of a vast variety of diseases or of special tendencies to them. Deaths by mere old age and the accompanying failure of vital powers without any w^ell defined malady, are very common in my collection, but I do not find as a rule, that the children of persons who die of old age have any marked immunity from specific diseases. There is a curious double appearance in the Eecords, 170 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. tlie one of an obvions liereditary tendency to disease and the other of the reverse. There are far too many striking instances of coincidence between the diseases of the parents and of the children to admit of reasonable doubt of their being often inherited. On the other hand, when I hide with my hand the lower part of a page such as those in Tables A and B, and endeavour to make a forecast of what I shall find under my hand after studying the upper portion, I am sometimes greatly mis- taken. Very unpromising marriages have occasionally led to good results, especially where the parental disease is one that usually breaks out late in life, as in the case of cancer. The children may then enjoy a fair length of days and die in the end of some other disease ; although if that disease had been staved off it is quite possible that the cancer would ultimately have appeared. I have two remarkable instances of this. In one of them, three grandparents out of four died of cancer. In each of the fraternities of which the father and mother were members, one and one person only, died of it. As to the children, although four of them have lived to past seventy years, not one has shown any sign of cancer. The other case differs in details, but is equally remarkable. However diseased the parents may be, it is of course possible that the children may inherit the healthier constitutions of their remoter ancestry. Pro- mising looking marriages are occasionally found to lead to a sickly progeny, but my materials are too scanty to permit of a thorough investigation of these cases. The general conclusion thus far is, that owing to X.] DISEASE. 171 the hereditary tendencies in each person to disease being usually very various, it is by no means always that useful forecasts can be made concerning the health of the future issue of any couple. Consumption. General Remarks. — The frequency of consumption in England being so great that one in at least every six or seven persons dies of it, and the fact that it usually appears early in life, and is therefore the less likely to be forestalled by any other disease, render it an appro- priate subject for statistics. The fact that it may be acquired, although there has been no decided hereditary tendency towards it, introduces no serious difficulty, being more or less balanced by the opposite fact that it may be withstood by sanitary precautions although a strong tendency exists. Neither does it seem worth while to be hypercritical and to dwell overmuch on the different opinions held by experts as to what constitutes consumption. The ordinary symptoms are patent enough, and are generally recognized ; so we may be content at first with lax definitions. At the same time, no one can be more strongly impressed than myself with the view that in proportion as we desire to improve our statistical work, so we must be in- creasingly careful to divide our material into truly homogeneous groups, in order that all the cases con- tained in the same group shall be alike in every important particular, difi"ering only in petty details. This is far more important than adding to the number 172 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. of cases. My material admits of no such delicacy of division ; nevertlieless it leads to some results worth mentioning. In sorting my cases, I included under the liead of Consumption all tlie causes of death described by one or the other following epithets, attention being also paid to the context, and to the phraseology used elsewhere by the same writer : — Consumption ; Phthisis ; Tuber- cular disease ; Tuberculosis ; Decline ; Pulmonary, or lung disease ; Lost lung ; Abscess on lung ; Haemorrhage of lungs (fatal) ; Lungs affected (here especially the context was considered). All of these were reckoned as actual Consumption. In addition to these there were numerous phrases of doubtful import that excited more or less reasonable suspicion. It may be that the disease had not suffi- ciently declared itself to justify more definite language, or else that the phrase employed was a euphemism to veil a harsh truth. Paying still more attention to the context than before, I classed these doubtful cases under three heads: — (1) Highly suspicious ; (2) Sus23i- cious ; (3) Somewhat suspicious. They were so rated that four cases of the first should be reckoned equivalent to three cases of actual consumption, four cases of the second to two cases, and four of the third to one case. The following is a list of some of the phrases so dealt with. The occasional appearance of the same phrase under different headings is due to differences in the context : — 1. Highly suspicious: — Consumptive tendency, Con- X.] DISEASE 173 sumption feared, and died of bad cliill. Chest colds with pleurisy and congestion of lungs. Died of an attack on the chest. Always delicate. Delicate lungs. Haemorrhage of lungs. Loss of part of lung. Severe pulmonary attacks and chest affections. 2. Suspicious : — Chest complaints. Delicate chest. Colds, cough and bronchitis. Delicate, and died of asthma. Scrofulous tendency. 3. Somewhat suspicious : — Asthma when young. Pul- monary congestion. Not strong ; anaemic. Delicate. Colds, coughs. Debility ; general weakness. [The con- text was especially considered in this group.] Parent to Child. — I have only four cases in which both parents were consumptive ; these will be omitted in the fol- lowing remarks ; but whether included or not, the results would be unaltered, for they run parallel to the rest. There are QQ marriages in which one parent was consumptive ; they produced between them 413 chil- dren, of whom 70 were actually consumptive, and others who were suspiciously so in various degrees. When reckoned according to the above method of computation, these amounted to 37 cases in addition, forming a total of 107. In other words, 26 per cent, of the children were consumptive. Where neither parent was consump- tive, the proportion in a small batch of well marked cases that I tried, was as high as 18 or 19 per cent, but this is clearly too much, as that of the general population is only 16 per cent. Again, by taking each fraternity separately and dividing the quantity of consumption in it by the number of its members, I obtained the averao;e 174 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. consumptive taint of each fraternity. For instance, if in a fraternity of 10 members there was one actually consumptive member and four "somewhat suspiciously" so, it would count as a fraternity of ten members, of whom two were actually consumptive, and the average taint of the fraternity would be reckoned at one-fifth part of the wdiole or as 20 per cent. Treating each fraternity separately in this way, and then averaging the whole of them, the mean taint of the children of one consumptive parent was made out to be 28 per cent. Distribution of Fraternities. — Next I arranged the fraternities in such way as would show whether, if we reckoned each fraternity as a unit, their respective amounts of consumptive taint were distributed ^'nor- mally " or not. The results are contained ia line A of the following table : — Percentage of Cases having various Percentages of Taint. Percentages of Taint. 0 and under 9 10 and under 19 20 and under 29 30 and under 39 40 and above Total. A. 66 cases, one parent con- sumptive. 27 20 9 15 29 100 B. 84 cases, one brother con- sumptive. 49 14 10 13 14 100 X.] DISEASE. 175 They struck me as so remarkable, in the way shortly to be explained, that I proceeded to verify them by as different a set of data as my Records could afford. I took every fraternity in which at least one member was consumptive, and treated them in a way that would answer the following question. " One member of a fraternity, whose number is unknown, is consumptive ; what is the chance that a named but otherwise un- known brother of that man will be consumptive also ? " The fraternity that was taken above as an example, would be now reckoned as one of nine members, of whom one was actually consumptive. There were 84 fraternities available for the present purpose, and the results are given in the line B of the table. The data in A and B somewhat overlap, but for the most part they differ. They concur in telling the same tale, namely, that it is totally impossible to torture the figures so as to make them yield the single-humped " Curve of Frequency " (Fig. 3 p. 38). They make a distinctly double-humped curve, whose outline is no more like the normal curve than the back of a Bactrian camel is to that of an Arabian camel. Consumptive taints reckoned in this way are certainly not " normally " distributed. They depend mainly on one or other of two groups of causes, one of which tends to cause complete immunity and the other to cause severe disease, and these two groups do not blend freely together. Consumption tends to be transmitted strongly or not at all, and in this respect it resembles the baleful influence ascribed to cousin 17G NATURAL INHERITANCE. [ciiap. marriages, which appears to be very small when statistically discussed, but of whose occasional severity most persons have observed examples. I interpret these results as showing that consumption is largely acquired, and that the hereditary influence of an acquired attack is small when there is no accom- panyiag "malformation." This last phrase is intended to cover not only a narrow chest and the like, but what- ever other abnormal features may supply the physical basis upon which consumptive tendencies depend, and which I presume to be as hereditary as any other malformations. Severely -tainted Fraternities. — Pursuing the matter further, I selected those fraternities in which consump- tion was especially frequent, and in which the causes of the deaths both of the Father and of the Mother were given. They were 14 in number, and contained be- tween them a total of 102 children, of whom rather more than half died before the age of 40. Though records of infant deaths were asked for, I doubt if they have been fully supplied.. As 102 differs little from 100, the following figures will serve as per- centages : 42 died of actual consumption and 11 others of lung disease variously described. Only one case was described as death from heart disease, but weakness of the heart during life was spoken of in a few cases. The remaining causes of death were mostly undescribed, and those that were named present no peculiarity worth notice. I then took out the causes of death of the x.| DISEASE. 177 Fathers and Mothers and their ages at death, and severally classified them as in the Table below. It must be understood that there is nothing in the Table to show how the persons were paired. The Fathers are treated as a group by themselves, and the Mothers as a separate group, also by themselves. Causes of Death of the Parents of those Fraternities in which Consumption Greatly Prevailed. Father ^S® ^* ^^^'^^^' death. Mother. ^^l^^ death. Order of ages at death. F. M. Asthma 70 Bronchitis 89 Inf. kidneys and bronchitis . 73 Abscess of liver through lung(alive) Heart 68 Heart 74 Apoplexy 62 Apoplexy 75 Apoplexy. . 78 Decay 74 Cancer 52 Senile gangrene 76 (2 bros, d. of cancer). Mortification of toe .... 59 Accident 51 (3 bros. and 2 ss. d. of cons.) Consumption 40 Consumption 43 Consumption 47 Consumption 55 Consumption 65 Consumption 66 Water on chest ■. . . .60 Weak chest .... (alive) (1 br. and 2 ss. d. of cons. ) Haemorrhage of lungs . 44 Ossification of heart . . 50 Nose bleeding 83 Cancer 42 Atrophy 73 Age 74 51 52 59 62 68 70 73 74 74 75 76 78 89 40 42 43 44 47 50 58 60 65 66 73 74 83 Very little account is given of the fraternities to which the fathers and mothers belong, and nothing of interest beyond what is included in the above. The contrast is here most striking between the tendencies of the Father and Mother to transmit a serious consumptive taint to their children. The cases were selected without the slightest bias in favour of showing this result ; in fact, such is the incapacity to see statistical facts clearly until they are pointed out, that 1 had no idea of the extraordinary tendency on 178 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. tlie part of tlie mother to transmit consumption, as sliown in this Table, until I had selected the cases and nearly finished sorting them. Out of the fourteen families, the mother was described as actually dying of consumption in six cases, of lung complaints in two others, and of having highly consumptive tendencies in another, making a total of nine cases out of the fourteen. On the other hand the Fathers show hardly any consumptive taints. One was described as of a very consumptive fraternity, though he himself died of an accident ; and another who was still alive had suffered from an abscess of the liver that broke through the lungs. Beyond these there is nothing to indicate consumption on the Fathers' side. Another way of looking at the matter is to compare the ag^es at death of the Mothers and of the Fathers respectively, as has been done at the side of the Table, when we see a notable difference between them, the Mid-age of the Mothers being 58, as against 73 of the Fathers. The only other group of diseases in my collection, that affords a fair number of instances in which frater- nities are greatly afiected, are those of the Heart. The instances are only nine in number, but I give an analysis of them, not for any value of their own, but in order to bring the peculiarities of the consumptive fraternities more strongly into relief by means of com- parison. In one of these there was no actual death from heart disease, though three had weak hearts and two others had rheumatic gout and fever. These nine X.] DISEASE. 179 families contained between them sixty-nine children, being at the rate of 7*7 to a family. The number of deaths from heart disease was 24 ; from ruptured blood vessels, 2 ; from consumption and lung disease, 8 ; from dropsy in various forms, 3 ; from apoplexy, paralysis, and epilepsy, 5 ; from suicide, 2 ; from Causes of Death of the Parents of those Fraternities in which Heart Disease Prevailed. Causes of death. Ages at death. Order of ages at death. Father. Mother. F. M. Heart Apoplexy and paralysis . Consumption Asthma Gout Senile Gangrene .... Tumour in liver .... Cancer Living Unknown 59, 70 74, 78 53 70 55 75 old. 61, 63, 74 62, 70, 72 si 77 85 2 bros. and 1 sis. d. of heart disease and 1 of paralysis cet. 40. 53 55 59 70 70 74 75 78 old. 61 62 63 70 72. 74 77 81 85 cancer, 1. There is no obvious difference between the diseases of their Fathers and Mothers as shown in the Table, other than the smallness of the number of cases would account for. Their mid-ages at death were closely the same, 70 and 72, and the ages in the two groups run alike. I must leave it to medical men to verify the amount of truth that may be contained in what I have deduced from these results concerning the distinctly superior N 2 180 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. power of the mother over that of the father to produce a highly consumptive family. Any physician in large practice among consumptive cases could test the ques- tion easily by reference to his note-books. A " highly consumptive " fraternity may conveniently be defined as one in which at least half of its members have actually died of consumption, or else are so stricken that their ultimate deaths from that disease may be reckoned upon. Also to avoid statistical accidents, the fraternities selected for the inquiry should be large, consisting say of six children and upwards. Of course the numerical proportions given by the above 14 frater- nities are very rude indications indeed of the results to which a thorough inquiry might be expected to lead. Accepting the general truth of the observation that consumptive mothers produce highly consumptive families much more commonly than consumptive fathers, it is easy to offer what seems to be an adequate ex- planation. Consumption is partly acquired by some form of contagion or infection, and is partly an here- ditary malformation. So far as it is due to the latter in the wide sense already given to the word " mal- formation," it may perhaps be transmissible equally by either parent. But so far as it is contagious or infectious, we must recollect that the child is pecu- liarly exposed during all the time of its existence before birth, to contagion from its mother. Daring infancy, it lies perhaps for hours daily in its mother's arms, and afterwards lives much by her side, closely caressed, and breathing the tainted air of her sheltered X.] DISEASE. 181 rooms. The ex23lanatioii of the fact that we have been discussing appears therefore to be summed u^ in the single word — ^Infection. Consumptivity . — Before abandoning the topic of hereditary consumption, it may be well to discuss it from the same point of view that was taken when discussing the artistic temperament. Consumption being so common in this country that fully one person out of every six or seven die of it, and all forms of hereditary disease being intermixed through marriage, it follows that the whole population must be n]ore or less tainted with consumption. That a condition which we may call '' consumptivity," for want of a better word, may exist without showing any outward sign, is proved by the fact that as sanitary conditions worsen by ever so little, more persons are affected by the disease. It seems a fair view to take, that when the amount of consumptivity reaches a certain level, the symptoms of consumption declare themselves ; that when it approaches but falls a little short of that level, there are threatening symptoms ; that when it falls far below the level, there is a fallacious appearance of perfect freedom from consumptivity. We may reason- ably proceed on the hypothesis that consumptivity might somehow be measured, and that if its measure- ment was made in each of any large group of persons, the measures would be distributed " normally." So far we are on fairly safe ground, but now un- certainties begin upon which my data fail to throw 182 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. sufficient light. Longevity, marriage, and fertility must all be affected by the amount of consumptivity, whereas in the case of the faculties hitherto discussed they are not affected to any sensible extent. It how- ever happens that these influences tend to neutralize one another. It is true that consumptive persons die early, and many of them before a marriageable age. On the other hand, they certainly marry earlier as a rule than others, one cause of which lies in their frequent great attractiveness ; and again, when they marry, they produce children more quickly than others. Consequently those who die even long before middle age, often contrive to leave large families. The greater rapidity with which the generations follow each other, is also a consideration of some importance. There is therefore a fair doubt whether a group of young persons destined to die of consumption, contribute considerably less to the future population than an equally large group who are destined to die of other diseases, I will at all events assume that consumptivity does not affect the numbers of the adult children, simply as a working hypothesis, and will afterwards compare its results with observed facts. I should add that the question whether the sexes transmit consumption equally, lies outside the present w^ork, at least for practical purposes ; for whether they transmit it equally or not would not affect the results materially. Our list of data is therefore limited to these: — that 16 per cent, of the population die of consumption, that consumptivity is normally distri- X.] DISEASE. 183 buted, and that tlie law of hereditary regression from a deviation of three units on the part of either parent to an average of one unit in the child, may be supposed to apply here, just as it did to Stature and to the other subjects of the preceding chapters. Let the scale by which consumptivity is measured be such that the Q of the general population = 1. Let its IVI = N, when measured on the same scale ; the value of N is and will remain unknown. Let N + C be the number of units of consumptivity that just amount to actual consumption. Our data tell us that 16 per cent, of the population have an amount of con- sumptivity that exceeds N + C. On referring to Table 8, w^e find the value of C that corresponds to the Grade of (100°— 16°), or of 84°, to be 1-47. There- fore whenever the consumptivity of a person exceeds N + 1*47, he has actual consumption. Adding together the tabular values in Table 8 at all the odd grades above 84°, we shall find their average value to be 2*23. We may therefore assume (see p. 160) that a group of persons each of whom has a consumpt- ivity of N + 2*23 will approximately represent all the grades above 84°. The Co-Fraternity descended . from such a group will have an IVI whose value according to the law of Eegression ought to be [N -f- ^ (2*23)] or [N + 0-74 units.] Those members of the Co-Fraternity are consumptive whose consumptivity exceeds N -j- 1*47 ; these are the same as those whose deviation from [N-l-0'74] which is the IVI of the Co-Fraternity, exceeds -h 073 unit. 184 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [cHAr. Let tlie Q of the Co-Fraternity be called n. The Grade at which this amount of deviation occurs should be found in Table 8 opposite to the value of 073 divided by n. Next as regards the value to be assigned to n, we may be assured that the Q of a Co-Fraternity cannot exceed that of the general population. Therefore n cannot exceed 1. In the case of Stature the relation between the Q of the Co-Fraternity and that of the Population was found to be as 1 5 to 1 7. If the same proportion held good here, its value would be 0"9. This is I think too high an estimate for the following reasons. The variability of the Co-Fraternity depends on two groups of causes. First, on fraternal variability ; which itself is due in part to mixed ancestry, and in part to variety of nurture in the same Fraternity, both before as well as after birth. Secondly, it depends upon the variety of ancestry and nurture in different Frater- nities. As to the first of the two groups of causes, they seem to affect consumptive fraternities in the same way as others, but not so with respect to the second group. The household arrangements of vigorous, of moderately vigorous, and of invalided parents are not alike. I have already spoken of infection. There is also a tradition in families that are not vio^orous, of the necessity of avoiding risks and of never entering professions that involve physical hardship. There is no such tradition in families who are vigorous. Thus there must be much greater variability in the environ- ments of a group of persons taken from the population X.] DISEASE. 185 at large, than there is in a group of consumptive families. It would be quite fair to estimate the value of n at least as low as 0'8. We have thus three values of n to try; viz. 1, 0*9, and 0*8, of which the first is scarcely possible and the last is much the more suitable of the other two. The corresponding values of 0'73 divided by 7i, are + 0.73, + O'Sl, and + 0'91. Eeferring to Table 8 we find the Grades corresponding to those deviations to be 69, 71, and 73. We should therefore expect 69, 71, or 73 per cent, of the Co-Fraternit}^ to be non-consumptive, according to the value of n we please to adopt, and the complement to those percentages, viz. 31, 29, or 27, to be consumptive. Observation (p. 173), gave the value of 26 by one method of calculation, and of 28 by another. Too much stress must not be laid on this coincidence, because many important points had to be slurred over, as already explained. Still, the prima facie result is successful, and enables us to say that so far as this evidence goes, the statistical method we have employed in treating consumptivity seems correct, and that the law of heredity found to govern all the difierent faculties as yet examined, appears to govern that of consump- tivity also, although the constants of the formula difi'er slightly. Data for Hereditary Diseases. — The knowledge of the officers of Insurance Companies as to the average value of unsound lives is by the confession of many of 186 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. x. them far from being as exact as is desirable. [See, for example, tlie discussion on a memoir by G. Humphreys, Actuary to the Eagle Insurance Company, read before the Institute of Actuaries. — Insur. Mag. xviii. p. 178.] Considering the enormous money value concerned, it would seem well worth the while of the higher class of those offices to combine in order to obtain a collec- tion of completed cases for at least two generations, or better still, for three ; such as those in Examples A and B, Appendix G, but much fuller in detail. Being com- pleted and anonymous, there could be little objection on the score of invaded privacy. They would have no perceptible effect on the future insurances of descend- ants of the families, even if these were identified, and they would lay the basis of a very much better knowledge of hereditary disease than we now possess, serving as a step for fresh departures. A main point is that the cases should not be picked and chosen to support any theor}", but taken as they come to hand. There must be a vast amount of good material in existence at the command of the medical officers of Insurance Companies. If it were combined and made freely accessible, it would give material for many years' work to competent statisticians, and would be certain, judging from all experience of a like kind, to lead to unexpected results. CHAPTER XL LATENT ELEMENTS. Latent Elements not very numerous. — Pure Breed. — Simplification of Hereditary Inquiry. Latent Elements not very numerous. — It is not possible tliat more than one half of the varieties and number of each of the parental elements, latent or personal, can on the average subsist in the off- spring. For if every variety contributed its repre- sentative, each child would on the average contain actually or potentially twice the variety and twice the number of the elements (whatever they may be) that were possessed at the same stage of its life by either of its parents, four times that of any one of its grandparents, 1024 times as many as any one of its ancestors in the 10th degree, and so on, which is absurd. Therefore as regards any variety of the entire inheritance, whether it be dormant or personal, the chance of its drojDping out must on the whole be equal to that of its being retained, and only one half of the varieties can on the average be passed on by inherit- 188 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. ance. Now we have seen that the personal heritage from either Parent is one quarter, therefore as the total heritage is one half, it follows that the Latent Elements must follow the same law of inheritance as the Personal ones. In other words, either Parent must contribute on the average only one quarter of the Latent Elements, the remainder of them dropping out and their breed becoming absolutely extinguished. There seems to be much confusion in current ideas about the extent to which ancestral qualities are transmitted, supposing that what occurs occasionally must occur invariably. If a maternal grandparent be found to contribute some particular quality in one case, and a paternal grandparent in another, it seems to be argued that both contribute elements in every case. This is not a fair inference, as will be seen by the following illustration. A pack of playing cards consists, as we know, of 13 cards of each sort — hearts, diamonds, spades, and clubs. Let these be shuffled together and a batch of 13 cards dealt out from them, forming the deal, No. 1. There is not a single card in the entire pack that may not appear in these 13, but assuredly they do not all appear. Again, let the 13 cards derived from the above pack, which we will suppose to have green backs, be shuffled with another 13 similarly obtained from a pack with blue backs, and that a deal. No. 2, of 13 cards be made from the combined batches. The result will be of the same kind as before. Any card of either of the two original packs may be found in the deal, No. 2, but certainly XL] LATENT ELEMENTS. 189 not all of tliem. So I conceive it to be with hereditary transmission. No given pair can possibly transmit the whole of their ancestral qualities ; on the other hand, there is probably no description of ancestor whose qualities have not been in some cases transmitted to a descendant. The fact that certain ancestral forms persist in breaking out, such as the zebra-looking stripes on the donkey, is no argument against this view. The reversion may fairly be ascribed to precisely the same cause that makes it almost impossible to wholly destroy the breed of certain weeds in a garden, inasmuch as they are prolific and very hardy, and wage successful battle with their vegetable competitors whenever they are not heavily outmatched in numbers. If the Personal and Latent Elements are transmitted on the average in equal numbers, it is difficult to suppose that there can be much difference in their variety. Pure Breed. — In a perfectly pure breed, maintained during an indefinitely long period by careful selection, the tendency to regress towards the M of the general population, would disappear, so far as that tendency may be due to the inheritance of mediocre ancestral qualities, and not to causes connected with the relative stability of different types. The Q of Fraternal Devia- tions from their respective true Mid-Fraternities which we called 6, would also diminish, because it is partly dependent on the children in the same family taking 190 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap variously after different and unlike progenitors. But tlie difference between 5 in a mixed breed such as we have been considering, and the value whicli we may call /3, wliich it would have in a pure breed, would be very small. Suppose the Prob: Error of the implied Stature of each separate Grand-Parent to be even as great as the Q of the general Population, which is 17 inch (it would be less, but we need not stop to discuss its precise value), then the Prob : Error of the implied Mid-Grand-Parental stature would be \/ J x 1'7 inch, or say 0*8 inch. The share of this, which would on the average be transmitted to the child, would be only ^ as much, or 0'2. From all the higher Ancestry, put together, the contribution would be much less even than this small value, and we may disregard it. It results that ¥ is a trifle greater than ^8^ 4-0*04. But 6 = 1'0; therefore 13 is only a trifle less than 0*98. Simplification of Hereditary Inquiry. — These considerations make it probable that inquiries into human heredity may be much simplified. They assure us that the possibilities of inheritance are not likely to differ much more than the varieties actually observed among the members of a large Fraternity. If then we have full life-histories of the Parents and of numerous Uncles and Aunts on both sides, we ought to have a very fair basis for hereditary inquiry. Information of this limited kind is incomparably more easy to obtain than that which I have hitherto striven for, namely, family histories during four successive generations. XI.] LATENT ELEMENTS. 191 When tlie " cliildren " in the pedigree are from 40 to 55 years of age, their own life-histories are sufficiently advanced to be useful, though they are incomplete, and it is still easy for them to compile good histories of their Parents, Uncles, and Aunts. Friends who knew them all would still be aliYC, and numerous documents such as near relations or personal friends preserve, but which are mostly destroyed at their decease, would still exist. If I were undertaking a fresh inquiry in order to verify and to extend my previous work, it would be on this basis. I should not care to deal with any family that did not number at least six adult children, and the same number of uncles and aunts on both the paternal and maternal sides. Whatever could be learnt about the grandparents and their brothers and sisters, would of course be acceptable, as throwing further light. I should how- ever expect that the peculiarities distributed among any large Fraternity of Uncles and Aunts would fairly indicate the variety of the Latent Elements in the Parent. The complete heritage of the child, on the average of many cases, might then be assigned as follows : One quarter to the personal characteristics of the Father ; one quarter to the average of the personal characteristics of the Fraternity taken as a whole, of whom the Father was one of the members; and similarly as regards the Mother's side. CHAPTEE XIL SUMMARY The investigation now concluded is based on tlie fact tliat the characteristics of any population that is in harmony with its environment, may remain statistically identical durino; successive orenerations. This is true for every characteristic whether it be affected to a great degree by a natural selection, or only so slightly as to be practically independent of it. It was easy to see in a vague way, that an equation admits of being based on this fact ; that the equation might serve to suggest a theory of descent, and that no theory of descent that failed to satisfy it could possibly be true. A large part of the book is occupied with preparations for putting this equation into a working form. Obstacles in the way of doing so, which I need not recapitulate, appeared on every side ; they had to be confronted in turns, and then to be either evaded or overcome. The final result was that the higher methods of statistics, which consist in applications of the law of Frequency of Error, were found eminently suitable for expressing CHAP. XII.] SUMMARY. 193 the processes of heredity. By their aid, the desired equation was thrown into an exceedingly simple form of approximative accuracy, and it became easy to compare both it and its consequences with the varied results of observation, and thence to deduce numerical results. A brief account of the chief hereditary processes occupies the first part of the book. It was inserted principally in order to show that a reasonable a priori probability existed, of the law of Frequency of Error being found to apply to them. It was not necessary for that purpose to embarrass ourselves with any details of theories of heredity beyond the fact, that descent either was particulate or acted as if it were so. I need hardly say that the idea, though not the phrase of particulate inheritance, is borrowed from Darwin's pro- visional theory of Pangenesis, but there is no need in the present inquiry to borrow more from it. Neither is it requisite to take Weissmann's views into account, unless I am mistaken as to their scope. It is freely conceded that particulate inheritance is not the only factor to be reckoned with in a complete theory of heredity, but that the stability of the organism has also to be regarded. This may perhaps become a factor of great importance in forecasting the issue of highly bred animals, but it was not found to exercise any sensible influence on those calculations with which this book is chiefly concerned. Its existence has therefore been only noted, and not otherwise taken into account. The data on which the results mainly depend had to be 0 194 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. collected specially, as no suitable material for the purpose was, so far as I know, in existence. This was done by means of an offer of prizes some years since, that placed in my hands a collection of about 160 useful Family Records. These furnished an adequate though only just an adequate supply of the required data. In order to show the degree of dependence that might be placed on them they were subjected to various analyses, and the result proved to be even more satisfactory than might have been fairly hoped for. Moreover the errors in the Records probably affect different generations in the same way, and would thus be eliminated from the comparative results. As soon as the character of the problem of Filial descent had become well understood, it was seen that a general equation of the same form as that by which it was expressed, also expressed the connection between Kins- men in every degree. The unexpected law of universal Regression became a theoretical necessity, and on appealing to fact its existence was found to be con- spicuous. If the word "peculiarity" be used to signify the difference between the amount of any faculty pos- sessed by a man, and the average of that possessed by the population at large, then the law of Regression may be described as follows. Each peculiarity in a man is shared by his kinsmen, but on the average in a less deg:ree. It is reduced to a definite fraction of its amount, quite independently of what its amount might be. The fraction differs in different orders of kinship, becoming smaller as they are more remote. When the XII.] SUMMARY. 195 kinsliip is so distant that its effects are not worth, taking into account, the peculiarity of the man, however re- markable it may have been, is reduced to zero in his kinsmen. This apparent paradox is fundamentally due to the greater frequency of mediocre deviations than of extreme ones, occurring between limits separated by equal widths. Two causes affect family resemblance ; the one is Heredity, the other is Circumstance. That which is transmitted is only a sample taken partly through the operation of "accidents," out of a store of otherwise un- used material, and circumstance must always play a large part in the selection of the sample. Circumstance comprises all the additional accidents, and all the pecu- liarities of nurture both before and after birth, and every influence that may conduce to make the characteristics of one brother differ from those of another. The circumstances of nurture are more varied in Co -Fra- ternities than in Fraternities, and the Grandparents and previous ancestry of members of Co-Fraternities differ; consequently Co-Fraternals differ among them- selves more widely than Fraternals. The average contributions of each separate ancestor to the heritage of the child were determined apparently within narrow limits, for a couple of generations at least. The results proved to be very simple; they assign an average of one quarter from each parent, and one sixteenth from each grandparent. According to this geometrical scale continued indefinitely back- wards, the total heritage of the child would be o 2 196 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap accounted for, but the factor of stability of type has to be reckoned witli, and this has not yet been adequately discussed. The ratio of filial Eegression is found to be so bound up with co-fraternal variability, that when either is given the other can be calculated. There are no means of deducing the measure of fraternal variability solely from that of co-fraternal. They differ by an element of which the value is thus far unknown. Consequently the measure of fraternal variability has to be calculated separately, and this cannot be done directly, owing to the small size of human families. Four different and indirect methods of attacking the problem suggested themselves, but the calculations were of too delicate a kind to justify reliance on the E.F.F. data. Separate and more accurate measures, suitable for the purpose, had therefore to be collected. The four problems were then solved by their means, and although different groups of these measures had to be used with the different problems, the results were found to agree together. The problem of expressing the relative nearness of different degrees of kinship, down to the point where kinship is so distant as not to be worth taking into account, was easily solved. It is merely a question of the amount of the Eegression that is appropriate to the different degrees of kinship. This admits of being directly observed when a sufficiency of data are acces- sible, or else" of being calculated from the values found in this inquiry. A table of these Eegressions was given. XII.] SUMMARY. 197 Finally, considerations were oflfered to show that latent elements probably follow the same law as personal ones, and that though a child may inherit qualities from any one of his ancestors (in one case from this one, and in another case from another), it does not follow that the store of hidden property so to speak, that exists in any parent, is made up of contributions from all or even very many of his ancestry. Two other topics may be mentioned. Eeason was given in p. 16 why experimenters upon the transmission of Acquired Faculty should not be dis- couraged on meeting with no affirmative evidence of its existence in the first generation, because it is among the grandchildren rather than among the children that it should be looked for. Again, it is hardly to be expected that an acquired faculty, if transmissible at all, would be transmitted without dilu- tion. It could at the best be no more than a variation liable to Eegression, which would probably so much diminish its original amount on passing to the grand- children as to render it barely recognizable. The difficulty of devising experiments on the transmission of acquired faculties is much increased by these considerations. The other subject to be alluded to is the funda- mental distinction that may exist between two couples whose personal faculties are naturally alike. If one of the couples consist of two gifted mem- bers of a poor stock, and the other of two ordinary members of a gifted stock, the difference between 198 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. xii. them will betray itself in their offspring. The children of the former will tend to regress ; those of the latter will not. The value of a good stock to the well- being of future generations is therefore obvious, and it is well to recall attention to an early sign by which we may be assured that a new and gifted variety possesses the necessary stability to easily originate a new stock. It is its refusal to blend freely with other forms. Some among the members of the same fraternity might possess the character- istics in question with much completeness, and the remainder hardly or not at all. If this alternative tendency was also witnessed among cousins, there could be little doubt that the new variety was of a stable character, and therefore capable of being easily developed by interbreeding into a pure and durable race. TABLES. Table 1. Strength of Pull. 519 Males aged 23-26. From mea sures made at the International Health Exhibition in 1884. Strength of Pull. No. of cases observed. Percentages. No. of cases observed. Sums from beginning. Under 50 lbs. 10 2 2 „ 60 ,, 42 8 10 „ 70 „ 140 27 37 ,, 80 „ 168 33 70 „ 90 „ 113 21 91 „ 100 „ 22 4 95 Above 100 ,, 24 5 100 Total 519 100 200 NATURAL INHERITANCE. _o -^ CO CN 0 00 0 CO OS '^ la C<1 Oi 0 «o CO ^ '^ l?q • -* CM CM ir^ (X) 0 -^ (io !>. -^ Oi 00 Oil 0 0 t^ CO CO CO CO j:^ ^ CO CO t^ CO r-l I— 1 CM 1-1 7-1 CM r-l o o 0 OT '^ x^^ CO CO t^ CO CO o • • X^i CM J>. t— Oi I— 1 0 l^ • CM iH Oi j-i i> t- iO CO CO CO '^H ir- t^ C30 iO 0 CO CM CO CO CO ^ rt X:^ 1:0 CO CO J>. CO rH l-H CM r-l 7-1 CM rH <1 o 0 CO T-l 05 CO ^ OS iH o • • • CO CO 00 ^ CM t^ XO CM • • 0 OS o o I— 1 o oo 0 iO CO CO CM >0 t^ CO 10 CO r-\ tH -* CO CM i-l 00 '"^ Oi CO 0 xo CM iH CO CM -g o (>? 0 t^ CO ^ IC 0 »o "73 o o • • • • • • 0 CM «0 7-4 0 '^ 7-1 00 CO !>. o J>. 05 -* CO ■<* CO CO 7— 1 -* t^ CO 10 CO I— I I— 1 CO 10 CM i-l 00 -^ Oi XO 0 -<* CM iH CM CM fii o 03 s iM 0 iO 05 CO - iH 0 aT O • • J>. Oi CO -* t^ CM 00 XO • • CO CO CO 00 CO «0 CO CO -^ CO CO 0 CO t^ CO '^ CM 7-1 I— 1 CM ^ CM " • XO '^ 0,00 *^oo .2 1^^ CO CO CD CO CO CO CO 05 CO CO CO ^ CM rH rH 7—1 CO CM T-t i:^ ^ 00 XQ CO CO tH tH CM CM (D I— 1 +3 ■■■" HB^^^^ CD a 0 CO Jr^ ^ CO 0 -* CO 00 o • • • • « 05 CO 7—1 7—1 7—1 00 CM 05 • • CO CM S o ^3 t^ CM CO CO IH) CO CO CO Oi CM CO CO CO 7-1 7-1 7-< 7-1 CO CM T-l t^ CO 00 -^ t-- (M iH iH CM CM o • r3 Pi IB !:§ 0 10 r-l CO CO cq t^ CM iH Co .Th :S o • 10 ^ Oi '^ 00 CO Oi t^ (M OS m-^ +3 CO CO CM kO CO QO r-l CO rH Oi CM CO CO i>. ^ CO cq CM iH S >^ rf CO CO CO CO CO CO I— 1 rH 1—1 1-4 7H r-l ^H M (U g'^ f3 00 CO 05 Oi cq t^ CM CO ■3 > 0 o CM 7-1 0 t-- U5 '::i4 -rjl CO CO 0 CO ^ § 10 r-l CO CO '^ CM CO CO t^ 0 CO CO CO 7—1 7-1 1-1 00 7—1 r-l T-l CO CO t^ ^ XO iH 7H r-< CM iH »p Oi 0 1—1 1—1 7—1 tH pi .2 ^ Ph l-H 0 CO iO l-H 0 0 CO CM ir^ Oi CO Oi CO CO rH CO CO 5? o 5rC 7—1 t^ .—1 t>. l-H t^ CM t^ T-l t^ 1-1 1^ 1—1 t--. tH t^ OS CO 6 2 n +- O 00 i>- 0 w 00 J:^ 0 CM CM CM XO CM XO CM XO (M CO '^ > c3 tw O o ^ ^^ 3 p ^0 rH , , . , . . . 1^ — ^-^ ^^— , — ' ^— v-- *— v-' —vi— — ^-i-- f3 O a;) W 0 CO CD m 0 a> 02 ft § 03 03 +f CO CD ^ r^ r^ rt 6a i=l Pi r^ S '5 a 0 0 0 pi pi pi •4-5 u 0 H f B fl rt i=l 0 0 0 Eej '^ P! h— 1 HH l-H 1— H Ph p^ l-H ft 1-4 T-i T-H CO CO c© «o CO CO --^ ^ .' .^ - ^ s c > ♦\ ^ J. >> ?5 ^* (U +3 ^ CO © ■ f=l f &c • ^•5 • •rH 03 ^ ■^ 5R 5R '^ S • o m o H i < c 5 3 s 0 ? 2 PJ ■^ (D C 0 ce .So- o _© X:^ IS ^ O o CO CM § O! -(-' ni 2 '^ PQ ;^ (D CO H (M PI O •rH r^ gj CO CO ^ O o m o M < l-l >• p 05 O CO'vtiCOlOCOCOOS-^OO^OSOO -^CO CC(M CMVO iJqC. laco a505t^ot;^C5(MC50cooo5-^ Oicrs t^°o i>-co J— ItHi— IC. t^OOJ>.00 1-^OOJ>-C5?0.00 COt>. l>.iO lOt^rHOCOt^OOOOi— lOt^ 050 CNCO ITiOO COCOMCOCOCO-^CDCqcO'^CN CO-:f< I^^CO (MICO O " * ' ' •'"' o CO o CM oooooooooooo oo oo >Ot^CO«DCO(NOOGvI?OOl-^ 050 ^00 COCO«OCOM1CO^-«*ICOCO-<*(N CO-<:t< COCO O OO lO CO 7-l^COCO(N05000COOCO 1>.?0 0-* OOt^:>.t>.OOCOOOl^OOt^GOiO t^^O GOOO (NCOlOC.ooa3i^<;o^ -hco ooco Oil— ioooioooooo<;ooooc30CD ooi>. «oi— i CO t— Oi cq 00 05 05 I— I J>. OO CO CO 05 O CO -* o o CO CO CM CM 00 oo ^£ C0rHCMV0i:0l0OO(M0i05(M CM>7-01>.QOJ?>-XOCMCM^ (A O !> .t^OOOOO(M Jr^OCSlOOOOOOOOOlOCM XO O O CM O CM 1— li— IOOCMr-tOi-(Ttl05J>.10 J>-1>. (Mi— I •S r5 ^ CD £ O PQ ^ S OJ O O M CO ^'^ r/5 ni 1 V) CI c3 P! p) 03 ^ §. 1-1 '-5 g=? j/2 o o c o PI as o OO CO C3» 202 NATURAL mHERITANCE. Tables 4 to 8 inclusive give data for drawing Normal Curves of Frequency and Distribution. They also show the way in which the latter is derived from the values of the Probability Integral. The equation for the Probablity Curve^ is y = k e in which A is "th-e Measure of Precision." By taking ^ and h each as unity, the values in Table 4 are computed. Table 4. Data for a Normal Curve of Frequency. y = e X y X y X y X y 0 1-00 ± 10 0-37 ± 2-0 0-0183 ± 3-0 0-0001 ± 0-2 0-96 ± 1-2 0 23 ± 2-2 0-0079 ± 0-4 0-85 ± 1-4 0 14 ± 2-4 0-0032 ± infi- 0-0000 ± 0-6 0-70 ± 1-6 0 78 ±2-6 0-0012 nity ± 0-8 0-53 ± 1-8 0-40 ± 2-8 0-0004 Table 5. 2 r* -f2 Values of the Probability Integral, — = I e cU, for Argument t. t{=hx) •0 •1 •2 -3 •4 •5 -6 •7 •8 -9 .1 2-0 infinite 0-00 0-843 -9953 1-0000 0-11 0-880 •9970 0-22 0 910 -9981 0-33 0-934 ■9989 0-43 0-952 •9993 0-52 0-966 -9996 0-60 0-976 •9998 0-68 0-984 9999 0-74 0-989 •9999 0-80 0-923 •9999 When t = '4769 the corresponding tabular entry would be '50; therefore, '4769 is the value of the " Probable Error." 1 See Merriman O71 the Method of Least Squares (Macmillan, 1885), pp. 26, 186, where fuller Tables than 4, 5, and 6 \Aill be found TABLES. 20: Table 6. Yalues of the Probability Integral for Argument q^^j^', that is, when the unit of measurement = the Probable error. Multiples of the •0 •1 •2 •3 •4 •5 •6 •7 •8 •9 Probable Error. 0 0-00 0-65 0-11 0-16 0-21 0-26 0-31 0-36 0-41 0-46 1-0 •50 •54 '58 •62 •66 •69 ••72 •75 •78 •80 2-0 •82 •84 •86 .88 •89 •91 •92 •93 •94 •95 3-0 •957 .964 •969 •974 •978 •982 •985 •987 •990 •992 4-0 •9930 .9943 •9954 •9963 •9970 •9976 •9981 •9985 •9988 •9990 5-0 infinite •9993 1^000 •9994 •9996 •9997 •9997 •9998 •9998 •9999 •9999 •9999 Tables 5 and 6 show the proportion of cases in any Normal system, in which the amount of Error lies within various extreme values, the total number of cases being reckoned as 1 'O. Here no re- gard is paid to the sign of the Error, whether it be plus or minus, but its amount is alone considered. The unit of the scale by which the Errors are measured, differs in the two Tables. In Table 5 it is the " Modulus," and the result is that the Errors in one half of the cases, that is in 0 50 of them lie within the extreme value (found by interpolation) of 0*4769, while the other half exceed that value. In Table 6 the unit of the scale is 0'4769. It is derived from Table 5 by dividing all the tabular entries by that amount. Consequently one half of the cases have Errors that do not exceed 1*0 in terms of the new unit, and that unit is the Probable Error of the System. It will be seen in Table 6 that the entry of "50 stands opposite to the argument of I'O. If it be desired to transform Tables 5 and 6 into others that shall show the proportion of cases in which the plus Errors and the minus Errors respectively lie within various extreme limits, their entries would have to be halved. Let us suppose this to have been done to Table 6, and that a new Table, which it is not necessary to print, has been thereby pro- duced and which we will call 6a. Next multiply all the entries in the new Table by 100 in order to make them refer to a total number of 100 cases, and call this second Table 6b. Lastly make a converse Table to 66 ; one in which the arguments of 6b become the entries, and the entries of 6b become the arguments. From this the Table 7 202 NATURAL INHERITANCE. is made. For example, in Table 6, opposite to the argument TOO, the entry of '50 is found ; that entry becomes "25 in 6a, and 25 in 66. In Table 7 the argument is 25, and the corresponding entry is 1-00. The meaning of this is, that in 25 per cent, of the cases the greatest of the Errors just attains to ± 1-0. Similarly Table 7 shows that the greatest of the Errors in 30 per cent, of the cases, just attains to ± 1"25 ; in 40 per cent, to 1*90, and so on. These various per- centages correspond to the centesimal Grades in a Curve of Distri- bution, when the Grade 0° is placed at the middle of the axis, which is the point where it is cut by the Curve, and where the other Grades are reckoned outwards on either hand, up to + 50° on the one side, and to — 50° on the other. To recapitulate : — In order to obtain Table 7 from the primary Table 5, we have to halve each of the entries in the body of Table 5, then to multiply each of the arguments by 100, and divide it by °4769. Then we expand the Table by interpolations, so as to include among its entries every whole number from 1 to 99 inclusive. Selecting these and disregarding the rest, we turn them into the arguments of Table 7, and we turn their corresponding arguments into the entries in Table 7. Table 7. Ordina-tes to Normal Curve of Distribution on a scale whose unit = the Probable Error ; and in which the 100 Grades run from 0° to + 50° on the one side, and to - 50° on the other. Grades. 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 0 0-00 0-04 0-07 0-11 0-15 0.19 0-22 0-26 0-30 0-34 10 0-38 0-41 0-45 0-49 0-53 0-57 0-61 0-65 0-69 0-74 20 0-78 0-82 0-86 0-97 0-95 1-00 1*05 1-10 1-15 1-20 30 3-25 1-30 1-36 1-42 1-47 1-54 1-60 1-67 1-74 1-82 40 1-90 1-99 2-08 2-19 2-31 2-44 2-60 2-79 3-05 3-45 But in the Schemes, the 100 Grades do not run from— 50° through 0° to + 50°, but from 0° to 100°. It is therefore convenient to modify Table 7 in a manner that will admit of its being used directly for drawing Schemes without troublesome additions or subtractions. This is done in Table 8, where the values from 50° onwards, and those from 50° backwards are identical with those in Table 7 from 0° to ± 50°, but the first half of those in Table 8 are positive and the latter half are negative. TABLES. 205 00 o Ph (O o o )^ o o H H I— I o O " c3 05 05 o CO CM 00 I— 1 O CO 1:- o CM 00 C5 rH 1 r— 1 I O 1 o 1 O 1 o +• o + rH + I— 1 + CO + CO o CO CO CO 00 o o CO C5 CO rH o CO 1 1— H 1 o I o 1 o 1 o 1 o + rH + 1-^ + CO + OS I— 1 1—1 05 as 1—1 CO CO o 1— 1 CO 05 1:^ CM 1 rH 1 o 1 o 1 o 1 o + o + I— 1 + I— < + CM + 1—1 CO 05 CO 1—1 cq CM 1—1 CO o o CO o CO CO I I— 1 1 o 1 o 1 o 1 o + o + I— 1 + rH + CM + o o 05 I— < C5 1—1 o o lO 1 I— i 1 1—1 1 o 1 o o + o + I— 1 + 1 — 1 + cq + O o CO XO o I— 1 CO C<1 Id 1 — 1 CO t- ^ 1 — 1 CO ^ 1 I— 1 1 1—i 1 o 1 o 1 o + o + o + 7— < CM + CO ir- 1 1:^ CO T— 1 I o I o 1 CO cq o 1 1—1 rH o + 05 6 + I— 1 o + r-H + Ci rH CM + o -* t- 1—1 CO o CO O CO GO CO CO 00 o (M CO 1 I— 1 I r— 1 1 o 1 o 1 o + o + o + I-H + CM + 00 o 1:- CO o rH 00 o CO 05 C5 T-H CO I I— 1 1 rH 1 o 1 o 1 o + o + o + I— 1 + rH + o 05 CM 00 1:- CO CO o o GO CO CO o C5 o 1 1—1 1 rH 1 O 1 o 1 o o + o + 1 — 1 + rH + o o T— 1 o o CO o o o CO o o 00 o o o tH >^ + +-> 1=1 •1-1 yd 4J o + »o x~-. OJ r^ ^ o o o ^ tH ^ 1 a a ,W r^ lO nC CM +-> -(-3 o o o iH (N (1) GO nri rt u 1 O tn aj ^ (D . IT tH O O) W -(J O O CO _o ."S ^ IS i-i ^ ^ d O " •S I Oi ^ ^ ^ oa O 0) fl o o 206 NATURAL INHERITANCE. Table 9. Marriage Selection in respect to Stature. The 205 male parents and the 205 female parents are each divided into three groups — T, M, and S, and t, m, and s, respectively — that is. Tall, Medium, and Stiort (medium male measurements being taken as 67 inches, and upwards to 70 inches). The number of marriages in each possible combination between them were then counted, with the result that men and women of contrasted heights, Short and Tall, or Tall and Short, married about as frequently as men and women of similar heights, both Tall or both Short ; there were 32 cases of the one to 27 of the other. s., t. 12 cases. M., t. 20 cases. T., t. 18 cases. S., m. 25 cases. M., m. 51 cases. T., m. 28 cases. S., s. 9 cases. M., s. 28 cases. T., s. 14 cases. Short and tall, 12 + 14 = 3 2 cases. Short and short, 9 \ _ ^,7 Tall and tall, 18 f - ^' ^^a^®^- We may therefore regard the married folk as couples picked out of the general population at haphazard when applying the law of probabilities to heredity of stature. Table 9a. Marriage Selection in respect to Eye- Colour in 78 Parental Couples. Eye Colour of No. of cases observed. Per Cents. 'Eije Colour of Husband and Wife. Husband Wife. Obs. Chance. Observed. Chance. Light Hazel Dark Light Hazel Dark 29 2 6 37 3 8 37 2 7 }" 46 Alike Light Hazel Hazel Dark Hazel Light Dark Hazel } - 1 ^ 23 5 15 7 V 28 22 / Half-con- \ trasted Light Dark Dark Light i " 24 32 24 32 Contrasted The chance combinations in pairs are calculated for a population containing 61*2 per cent, of Light Eye-colour, 12-7 of Hazel, and 26-1 of Dark. TABLES. 207 Table 9 b. Marriages of the Artistic and the Not Artistic. Rank in Pedigrees. No. of per- sons. Percentages. Pairs of artistic and not artistic persons. Marriages observed. Chance combinations. art. , not. art. not. both art. 1 art. Inot. both not. both art. 1 art. Inot. both not. Parents 326 280 288 32 27 24 68 73 76 39 30 28 61 70 72 14 12 9 31 31 41 50 57 50 12 8 7 46 41 39 42 51 54 Paternal grandparents.. Maternal grandparents.. Totals and means... 894 28 72 33 67 12 36 52 9 42 49 Tastes of Husband and Wife — alil^'ft 12 -f 52 =64 36 9 + 49 = 58 42 ( 3ontrastp.d Table 10. Effect upon Adult Children of Differences in Height of their Parents. Difference in inches between the Heights of the Parents. Proportion per 50 of cases in which the Heights^ of the Children deviated to various amounts from the Mid-filial Stature of their respective families. Number of Children whose Heights were observed. (Total 525. ) Less than 2 inches. ^CO Cj (X) Under 1 inch 21 23 16 24 18 35 37 34 35 30 43 46 41 41 40 46 49 45 47 47 48 50 49 49 49 105 122 112 108 78 1 and under 2 2 „ 3 3 „ 5 , 5 and above ^ Every female height has been transmuted to its male equivalent by multi- plying it byl"08, and only those families have been included in which the number of adult children amounted to six, at least. j^OTE. — When these figures are protracted into curves, it will be seen — (1) that they run much alike ; (2) that their peculiarities are not in sequence ; and (3) that the curve corresponding to the first line occupies a medium position. It is therefore certain that differences in the heigh ts of the Parents have on the whole an inconsiderable effect on the heights of their Offspring. 208 NATURAL INHERITANCE. p4 ^ '- o o ^ i=l 4^ d o <1 00 to CO CO O I— I O ni -^ . -1-3 "3 M m 'S S 2 3 C^OS1005(Mi»CvIJ>.00 CMC5iCiOOOOt~^i--.CC>lO i>- O?D?0i^?0C0C0C0 Oi:07-IC.Oi-*>— I'*'* •!— I (M -!tl t^ O CO T-l -CM (M I— I r-i '• r-IO-^lOr-HOi-^lO 1— I T— I CM . :i-H j-H G<1 CO CM I— I '^t^co^>-I-^C50^^-.l— lOCM CM CO CO rH 1—1 C0r-IJ>-^O<^l>.r-l»O(M rH CM CO I— I T-H 7— li— l-*eO»r5CMJ>.rHi— I «0 I— I ^ O O -* "* r-irHl>.»r5COOS-^(M (MC^7^00iOOl^«OiO^* x>.i>.j>.i>.co«o«0':Di:o o o I — I OS t--. iO CS 33 r;:^ rC3 ■ o fH ; O) _0 r^ fl ^ cj o S ^ - «2 S 'S o-i 3 rH S =« O o -4-) ^ .P5 fl'tn -+J +? liM > r 2 o OJ 03 rc3. 5=^.52 S > S f-i lO o "^ "' si "J Pi «3 >. ■+^ ?o fl Eh -M Ti ttH d <^ d o S «« W rd 03 ^ S S «! 1:3 ce 03 CS o fl f-l ^ d _ O P^ W H O pet o P5 m t> Eh m CO CO CO CO 05 i:^ CM CM CO •i-H o Oi Oi CO OO !>. t^ t^ t^ !>. t^ T-\ • ^ 1— 1 T-^ 1— 1 JC^ CO (M >-l 1-1 '^ CO CO CO 00 O CO Ml T-i T-i : icO ® T-i r-i rH CO w <© (M 1© ; • <>4 ^ -* CO CO CO J>. -* CO : •" 05 CO (M CO ; tH * (M 'stl t -^ rH r-i \ \ CO r-i cq (N ; : ; ; T-i rH (M CO 1- ; r-i tH T-i ; r-\ T-i • ; ', : : VO ° rt S • (N cq cq oi (?q (M CM CM CM CM CM cq J>1 CQ 03 rj CO o:> (N tH o 05 OO 1-^ CO xo 4ti 00 CM 1—1 > o i:^ t>. t>. ir^ CO CO CO CO CO CO CO CO CO o 1 — 1 <1 w 210 NATURAL INHERITANCE. o3 o3 c5 o CO )— I pa 1-1 m W H O pq H w O W 2 o o M 1^ P3 J5 2 ^ H-1 t3 13 o M -a; 1> o CO p^ w M H O P^ o P5 pq ;^5 . i^ CO lO t^ t^ CO CO CO CO CO CO CO g 4^ S ^ t^ x^ oo tH CO CO OS iO o '^ o CO OS CM cq ^ OO J>. OS o OS »o 1~i CO (N CN CM O C3 I— 1 1-1 (N 1—1 r-i 1—1 CO H « 1—1 CD 1^ J (M OJ 1- 1—1 M OS I— ^ o cc (>J I— 1 o CO . T-^ (N CO eo CO CO o O CO CN rCj lO b£) T— rN t^ 1— oc \a a: cc o CC -^ (N CO 1— I— CO p T-H 7-1 T-i OS CO CO oo lO CO CO o ICl ^ OJ CO 1— r-^ J-H CO 1-H lO T-1 (N lO 00 o iO (M 00 CO ^ ^ 1— r- 1- CO CO IP J— 1 r-l tH tH "^ CO co oc CM VO OS CO CM CO 1? ^00 1—1 G O 0-" . r^^ as \r. ^ cc \C c 05 oc cc in cc cc c J a- a C 1— 1 tj t^ J> J> I> . J> CC CO co cc cc cc cc a; P5 TABLES. 211 Table 14 (Special Data). Deviations of individual Brothees from their Mid-Fraternal Statures. Number of brothers in eacli family 4 5 6 7 Number of Families 39 23 8 6 Amount of Deviation. Number of cases. Number of cases. Number of cases. Number of cases. Under 1 inch 88 49 15 4 62 30 17 3 3 20 18 5 3 2 21 14 6 1 1 and under 2 2 and under 3 3 and under 4 4 and above P 2 212 NATURAL INHERITANCE. vo fe H pq c o C o K bJD -M p CD CO o OS 05 p O Oi Oi O o OS 05 O O 7—1 i-i p o o T-l OS p 7^ o OS O O OS OS O O OS 7—1 7—1 p Oi <3S O O O O o o o o 7— 1 7— ( 7— 1 T— 1 CM o o T-l •8 «p i^ CX) O (jq CO 4t4 05 p CO (M CO CO ^~ -* CM CO i-l p CM CO CO -^ CM p CO •UA\.oiq 2[.reQ[ 'I o <^ o -* Oi CM O O 7— 1 I—I I—I OO o 7—1 p Xp CM i^^ CM CM 00 i:^ 7—1 7—1 XO o 7—1 7-1 CM Oi OS 7—1 7—1 1^ o 7—1 •nAVoig •9 ^ _, <^ ^ 00 o ""I cq 7— 1 "^ 1— 1 o 7—1 p O CO lO lO CO O 7— 1 7— I 7—1 tH 7—1 CM I— 1 CM CM 7-1 (M 7—1 I— 1 7—1 7—1 p 7—1 7—1 •nA^.ojq ^qSi^ •s ■tH TjH i-i CO o o Ah o o -^ p -tH t^ O O O 7^ p O 7—1 7—1 7—1 7—1 o •pzujj "^QJO -Fi^a •t «p cs cp t^ 00 O -*l VO )— 1 I— 1 r-1 CO 1 CO OS 7— 1 7—1 7—1 7- H CO 7—1 O 7-1 -* OO T-l 7—1 7—1 7-H CM 7—1 •na9iS-9ni;g '^s^i^ •8 -^ 7^ 00 -^ OS o 7— 1 1— (M CO CO CM (M O CO (^o p cx) j>. kO CM CM CO ^ o o w a; CO o O d ■i o CO CO . t^ CM CM CM O CM m CD lO ^ CO OO ^ J:^ t^ (M CO 7—1 CM CM CO XO J^^ XO 7— 1 7— 1 t^ OO Oi XO ^* XO 7— t r-1 O OS •UAVoiq :j[i'Bp Xt9^ '8 (M Ttl 1^ OS r-i CM CO 7—1 00 OS t^ ^ XO I— 1 7— i CM CD 7—1 7—1 O '^ CO ^ CD 7-1 CD ^# 7—1 •uMo.ig :5[j«q; •z -«d< t^ ^ 7— 1 •^ OS J>. CO CM 00 COO CM XO OS CD CM CO 00 CM CM O ^ CO O OS Ot) XO 7 1 1 ^7—1 Oi •UAioig; '9 OS OS CO J>- co J:^ OO CO 00 CO CM O O 00 CO t- O t^ CO T-H 7—1 00 CM OS OS 7—1 o O J>- CD t-^ 7—17—17-1 OS 7—1 XO •iiMOjq ^^qSil ■9 (M CO 00 rH 7—1 CM t^ CO VO 7—1 ■"^ O 7-H CO 7—1 T-l 7—1 CO •pz^H 'IdiS :^i-E(j 'f o ^ CO i-» ^ CO o ^ 7—1 <:j5 CM OO Oj C30 vo ^ OO C3S XO o OS CM OO CO CD CM 00 t^ O O 7-1 CM 7-1 OS CD XO •n99cig-9h{g; 'Adi{) •8 1^ CO «0 (M 00 CO CO OO 7-1 CM CM OO •^ 7-1 7-1 CM 7— 1 ^# '^ 00 7-1 CM CM OO CD O T^ t^ ^ -* t-- xr^ CD CM rtl -^ r-l XO CD CO T-l •9n|q ^,n3Q[ -gnig •5 t^ ^ti 1^ OS t^ CO CO 00 7-( CM 7— 1 00 CO CM CO O 00 CO t^ 7— 1 J:^ 7—1 7—1 CM CO Oi XO OS j>. i>. j>. O O tr^ CO CO ^ CO 7-1 O CD CM 7—1 •9n[q :iqSil '1 CO OS O CO 7—1 7—1 CO CO t^ CM 7-1 CO CM CM CD XO O ^^ 7— 1 OS CM -* XO 7—1 CM T— 1 Sex and the No. of the (ascending) generation. HH 1— 1 1— 1 H- «3 Is 1— < 1 — 1 >— 1 1— 1 1— 1 Is B 1^ K* 1— 1 H- i : HH Hi I—I l-H CO CO r-H 1 _ci^ /-1 1 \ t^^^~~¥ ^ ^ Prohlem 2. — An index q moves under some restraint up and down a bar AQB, its mean position for any given position of the bar 224 NATURAL INHERITANCE. being Q ; the bar, always carrying tbe index with it, moves under some restraint up and down a fixed frame YMY', the mean position of Q being M : the movements of the index relatively to the bar and of the bar relatively to the frame being quite independent. For any given observed position of q, required the most probable position of Q fwhich cannot be observed) ; it being known that the probable error of q relatively to Q in all positions is h, and that of Q rela- tively to M is G. The ordinary law of error is to be assumed. If in any one observation, MQ = x, Q,q = y^ then the law of error requires ^ + r (12) to be a minimum, subject to the condition X + y = a, 2^ constant. Hence we have at once, to determine the most probable values of and the most probable position of Q, measured from M, when ^'s ob- served distance from M is a, is h^ + c2 ""' It also follows at once that the probable error i? of Q (which may be obtained by substituting a—xiovy in (12)) is given by 11,1 ^« V^ G^ 0^ V62_j_c2 ^ ^ which it is important to notice, is the same for all values of a. APPENDIX C. 223 C. EXPERIMENTS ON SWEET PEAS BEARING ON THE LAW OF REGRESSION. The reason why Sweet Peas were chosen, and the methods of selectiug and planting them are described in Chapter YI., p. 79. The following Table justifies their selection by the convenient and accu- rate method of weighing, as equivalent to that of measuring them. It will be seen that within the limits of observed variation a difference of 0*172 grain in weight corresponds closely to an average difference of O'Ol inch in diameter. Table 1. Comparison of Weights of Sweet Peas with their Diameters. Distinguishing letter of "Weight of one seed in grains. Length of row of 100 seeds in Diameter of one seed in hundredths of inch. seed. Common difference = 0'172 grain. inches. Common difference = 0 01 inch. K 1-750 21-0 21 L 1-578 20-2 20 M 1-406 19-2 19 N r234 17-9 18 0 1-062 17-0 17 P •890 16-1 16 Q •718 15-2 15 The results of the experiment are given in Table 2 ; its first and last columns are those that especially interest us ; the remaining columns showiog how these two were obtained. It will be seen that for each increase of one unit on the part of the parent seed, there is a mean increase of only one-third of a unit in the filial seed ; and again that the mean filial seed resembles the parental when the latter is about 15*5 hundredths of an inch in diameter. Taking 15-5 as the point towards which Filial Hegression points, whatever may be the parental deviation from that point, the mean Filial Deviation will be in the same direction, but only one- third as much. Q 226 NATURAL INHERITANCE. Table 2. Parent Seeds and their Produce. Tlie proportionate number of sweet peas of diififerent sizes, produced by parent seeds also of different sizes, are given below. The measurements are those ot their mean diameters, in hundredths of an inch. Diameter of Parent Diameters of Filial Seeds. Total. Mean Diameter of Filial Seeds. Seed. Under 15. 15- 16- 17- 18- 19- 20- Above 21- Observed Smoothed 21 22 8 10 18 21 13' 6 2 100 17-5 17-3 20 23 10 12 17 20 13 3 2 100 17-3 17-0 19 35 16 12 13 11 10 2 1 100 16-0 16 6 IS 34 12 13 17 16 6 2 0 100 16-3 16-3 17 37 16 13 16 13 4 1 0 100 15-6 16-0 16 34 15 18 16 13 3 1 0 100 16-0 15-7 15 46 14 9 11 14 4 2 0 100 15-3 15-4 This point is so low in the scale, that I possess less evidence than I desired to prove the bettering of the produce of very small seeds. The seeds smaller than Q were such a miserable set that I could hardly deal with them. Moreover, they were very infertile. It did, however, happen that in a few of the sets some of the Q seeds turned out very well. If I desired to lay much stress on these experiments, I could make my case considerably stronger by going minutely into other details, including confirmatory measurements of the foliage and length of pod, but I do not care to do so. D. GOOD AND BAD TEMPER IN ENGLISH FAMILIES. One of the questions put to the compilers of the Family Records spoken of in page 72, referred to the "Character and Tempera- ment " of the persons described. These were distributed through ^ Eeprinted after slight revision from FortnigMly R&view, July, 1887. APPENDIX D. 227 three and sometimes four generations, and consisted of those who lay in the main line of descent, together with their brothers and sisters. Among the replies, I find that much information has been incidentally included concerning what is familiarly called the "temper" of no less than 1,981 persons. As this is an adequate number to allow for many inductions, and as temper is a strongly marked characteristic in all animals ; and again, as it is of social interest from the large part it plays in influencing domestic hap- piness for good or ill, it seemed a proper subject for investigation. The best explanation of what I myself mean by the word " temper" will be inferred from a list of the various epithets used by the compilers of the Kecords, which I have interpreted as expressing one or other of its qualities or degrees. The epithets are as follows, arranged alphabetically in the two main divisions of good and bad temper : — Good temjjer. — Amiable, buoyant, calm, cool, equable, forbearing, gentle, good, mild, placid, self-controlled, submissive, sunny, timid, yielding. (15 epithets in all.) Bad tem^oer. — Acrimonious, aggressive, arbitrary, bickering, capricious, captious, choleric, contentious, crotchety, decisive, de- spotic, domineering, easily offended, fiery, fits of anger, gloomy, grumpy, harsh, hasty, headstrong, huffy, impatient, imperative, im- petuous, insane temper, irritable, morose, nagging, obstinate, odd- tempered, passionate, peevish, peppery, proud, pugnacious, quarrel- some, quick-tempered, scolding, short, sharp, sulky, sullen, surly, uncertain, vicious, vindictive. (46 epithets in all.) I also grouped the epithets as well as I could, into the following five classes : 1, mild ; 2, docile ; 3, fretful ; 4, violent ; 5, masterful. Though the number of epithets denoting the various kinds of bad temper is three times as large as that used for the good, yet the number of persons described under the one general head is about the same as that described under the other. The first set of data that I tried, gave the proportion of the good to the bad-tempered as 48 to 52 ; the second set as 47 to 53. There is little difference between the two sexes in the frequency of good and bad temper, but that little is in favour of the women, since about 45 men are re- 2 Q 228 NATURAL INHERITANCE. corded as good-tempered for every 55 wlio are bad, and conversely 55 women as good-tempered for 45 who are bad. I will not dwell on the immense amount of unhappiness, ranging from family discomfort down to absolute misery, or on the breaches of friendship that must have been occasioned by the cross-grained, sour, and savage dispositions of tho^e who are justly labelled by some of the severer epithets ; or on the comfort, peace, and good- will diffused through domestic circles by those who are rightly described by many of the epithets in the first group. We can hardly, too, help speculating uneasily upon the terms that our own relatives would select as most appropriate to our particular selves. But these considerations, interesting as they are in themselves, lie altogether outside the special purpose of this inquiry. In order to ascertain the facts of which the above statistics are a brief summary, I began by selecting the larger families out of my lists, namely, those that consisted of not less than four brothers or sisters, and by noting the persons they included who were described as good or bad-tempered ; also the remainder about whose temper nothing was said either one way or the other, and whom perforce I must call neutral. I am at the same time well aware that, in some few cases a tacit refusal to describe the ^temper should be inter- preted as reticence in respect to what it was thought undesirable even to touch upon. I found that out of a total of 1,361 children, 321 were described as good-tempered, 705 were not described at all, and 342 were described as bad-tempered. These numbers are nearly in the pro- portion of 1, 2, and 1, that is to say, the good are equal in number to the bad-tempered, and the neutral are just as numerous as the good and bad-tempered combined. The equality in the total records of good and bad tempers is an emphatic testimony to the correct judgments of the compilers in the choice of their epithets, for whenever a group has to be divided into three classes, of which the second is called neutral, or medium, or any other equivalent term, its nomenclature demands that it should occupy a strictly middlemost position, an equal number of con- trasted cases flanking it on either hand. If more cases were recorded of good temper than of bad, the compilers would have laid down the boundaries of the neutral zone unsymmetrically, too far APPENDIX D. 229 from the good end of the scale of temper, and too near the bad end. If the number of cases of bad temper exceeded that of the good, the error would have been in the opposite direction. But it appears, on the whole, that the compilers of the records have erred neither to the right hand nor to the left. So far, therefore, their judgments are shown to be correct. ISText as regards the proportion between the number of those who rank as neutrals to that of the good or of the bad. It was recorded as 2 to 1 ; is that the proper poportion 1 Whenever the nomencla- ture is obliged to be somewhat arbitrary, a doubtful term should be interpreted in the sense that may have the widest suitability. Now a large class of cases exist in which the interpretation of the word neutral is fixed. It is that in which the three grades of magnitude are conceived to result from the various possible combinations of two elements, one of which is positive and the other negative, such as good and bad, and which are supposed to occur on each occasion at haphazard, but in the long run with equal frequency. The number of possible combinations of the two elements is only four, and each of these must also in the long run occur with equal frequency. They are: 1, both positive; 2, the first positive, the second negative ; 3, the first negative, the second positive ; 4, both negative. In the second and third of these combinations the negative counterbalances the positive, and the result is neutral. Therefore the proportions in which the several events of good, neutral, and bad would occur is as 1, 2, and 1. These proportions further commend themselves on the ground that the whole body of cases is thereby divided into two main groups, equal in number, one of which includes all neutral or medium cases, and the other all that are exceptional. Probably it was this latter view that was taken, it ma,y be half unconsciously, by the compilers of the Eecords. Anyhow, their entries conform excellently to the proportions speci- fied, and I give them credit for their practical appreciation of what seems theoretically to be the fittest standard. I speak, of course, of the Records taken as a whole ; in small groups of cases the proportion of the neutral to the rest is not so regular. The results shown in Table I. are obtained from all my returns. It is instructive in many ways, and not least in showing to a statistical eye how much and how little value may reasonably be 230 NATURAL INHERITANCE. attached to my materials. It was primarily intended to discover whether any strong bias existed among the compilers to spare the characters of their nearest relatives. In not a few cases they have written to me, saying that their records had been drawn up with perfect frankness, and earnestly reminding me of the importance of not allowing their remarks to come to the knowledge of the persons described. It is almost needless to repeat what I have published miore than once already, that I treat the Records quite confidentially. I have left written instructions that in case of my death they should all be destroyed unread, except where I have left a note to say that the compiler wished them returned. In some instances I know that the Records were compiled by a sort of family council, one of its members acting as secretary ; but I doubt much whether it often happened that the Records were known to many of the members of the family in their complete form. Bearing these and other con- siderations in mind, I thought the best test for bias would be to divide the entries into two contrasted groups, one including those who figured in the pedigrees as either father, mother, son, or daughter— that is to say, the compiler and those who were very nearly related to him — and the other including the uncles and aunts on both sides. Table 1. Distribution of Temper in Families (per cents.) Relationships. 3 r-H o o ft 1 CD CO 1 — 1 o m O CO o t O CQ a. Fathers and Sons &. Mothers and Daughters c. Uncles d. Aunts a + b. Direct line 35 39 32 39 74 71 12 18 13 14 30 27 32 31 25 29 63 54 12 8 18 9 20 27 9 4 12 9 13 21 100 100 100 100 200 200 188 179 272 238 367 510 c + d. Collaterals Good. Bad Temper. 200 200 367 510 a + b. Dhect line 10^ Qfi c + d. Collaterals 9 8 102 APPENDIX D. 231 On compariDg the entries, especially tlie summaries in the lower lines of the Table, it does not seem that the characters of near relatives are treated much more tenderly than those of the more remote. There is little indication of the compilers having been biased by affection, respect, or fear. More cases of a record being left blank when a bad temper ought to have been recorded, would probably occur in the direct line, but I do not see how this could be tested. An omission may be due to pure ignorance ; indeed I find it not uncommon for compilers to know very little of some of their uncles or aunts. The Records seem to be serious and careful com- positions, hardly ever used as vehicles for personal animosity, but written in much the same fair frame of mind that most people force themselves into when they write their wills. Table 2. Combinations of Temper in Marriage (per cents.). Tempers of Husbands. A. — Observed Pairs. B. — Haphazard Pairs. Tempers of Wives. Tempers of Wives. Good. Bad Tempers. Good. Bad Tempers, 1 2 3 4 5 2 2 1 1 1 13 5 11 6 4 2 3 4 5 Good 1 „ 2 6 4 10 2 9 5 6 2 5 2 10 4 3 1 2 1 2 1 1 Bad 3 „ 4 „ 5 14 7 3 4 3 9 3 2 3 2 5 2 2 8 5 3 2 1 1 Good 22 24 25 21 Bad 3 1 23 3 0 24 The sexes are separated in the Table, to show the distribution of the five classes of temper among them severally. There is a large proportion of the violent and masterful among the men, of the fretful, the mild, and the docile among the women. On adding the entries it will be found that the proportion of those who fall 232 NATURAL INHERITANCE. within the several classes are 36 per cent, of mild-tempered, 15 per cent, of docile, 29 per cent, of fretful, 12 per cent of violent, 8 per cent, of masterful. The importance assigned in marriage-selection to good and bad temper is an interesting question, not only from its bearing on domestic happiness, but also from the influence it may have in promoting or retarding the natural good temper of our race, assum- ing, as we may do for the moment, that temper is hereditary. I caDnot deal with the~ question directly, but will give some curious facts in Table II. that throw indirect light upon it. There a comparison is made of (A) the actual frequency of marriage between persons, each of the various classes of temper, with (B) the calculated fre- quency according to the laws of chance, on the supposition that there had been no marriage-selection at all, but that the pairings, so far as temper is concerned, had been purely at haphazard. There are only 111 marriages in my lists in which the tempers of both parents are recorded. On the other hand, the number of possible combinations in couples of persons who belong to the five classes of temper is very large, so I make the two groups comparable by reducing both to percentages. It will be seen that with two apparent exceptions in the upper left-hand corners of either Table (of 6 against 13, and of 10 against 5), there are no indications of predilection for, or avoidance of marriage between persons of any of the five classes, but that the figures taken from observation run as closely with those derived through calculation, as could be expected from the small number of observations. The apparent exceptions are that the percentage of mild-tempered men who marry mild-tempered women is only 6, as against 13 calculated by the laws of chance, and that those who marry docile wives are 1 0, as against a calculated 5. There is little difference between mildness and docility, so we may throw these entries together without much error, and then we have 6 and 10, or 16, as against 13 and 5, or 18, which is a close approximation. We may compare the frequency of marriages between persons of like temper in each of the five classes by reading the Table diagonally. They are as (6), 2, 9, 2, 1, in the observed cases, against (13), 2, 8, 1, 1, in the calculated ones; here the irregularity of the 6 and 13, which are put in brackets for distinction sake, is APPENDIX D. 233 conspicuous. Elsewhere there is not the slightest indication of a dislike in persons of similar tempers, whether mild, docile, fretful, violent, or masterful, to marry one another. The large initial figures 6 and 13 catch the eye, and at a first glance impress them- selves unduly on the imagination, and might lead to erroneous speculations about mild tempered persons, perhaps that they find one another rather insipid ; but the reasons I have given, show conclu- sively that the recorded rarity of the marriages between mild-tempered persons is only apparent. Lastly, if we disregard the five smaller classes and attend only to the main divisions of good and bad temper, there does not appear to be much bias for, or against, the marriage of good or bad-tempered persons in their own or into the opposite division. The admixture of different tempers among the brothers and sisters of the same family is a notable fact, due to various causes which act in different directions. It is best to consider them before we proceed to collect evidence and attempt its interpretation. It becomes clear enough, and may be now taken for granted, that the tempers of progenitors do not readily blend in the offspring, but that some of the children take mainly after one of them, some after another, but with a few threads, as it w^ere, of various ancestral tempers woven in, which occasionally manifest themselves. If no other influences intervened, the tempers of the children in the same family would on this account be almost as varied as those of their ancestors ; and these, as we have just seen, married at haphazard, so far as their tempers were concerned ; therefore the numbers of good and bad children in families would be regulated by the same laws of chance that apply to a gambling table. But there are other influences to be considered. There is a well-known tendency to family likeness among brothers and sisters, which is due, not to the blending of ancestral peculiarities, but to the prepotence of one of the progenitors, who stamped more than his or her fair share of qualities u.pon the descendants. It may be due also to a familiar occurrence that deserves but has not yet received a distinctive name, namely, where all the children are alike and yet their common likeness cannot be traced to their progenitors. A new variety has come into existence through a process that affects the whole Frater- nity and may result in an unusually stable variety (see Chapter III.). The most strongly marked family type that I have personally met 234 NATURAL INHERITANCE. with, first arose simultaneously in the three brothers of a family who transmitted their peculiarities with unusual tenacity to numer- ous descendants through at least four generations. Other influences act in antagonism to the foregoing ; they are the events of domestic life, which instead of assimilating tempers tend to accentuate slight differences in them. Thus if some members of a family are a little submissive by nature, others who are naturally domineering are tempted to become more so. Then the acquired habit of dictation in these reacts upon the others and makes them still more sub- missive. In the collection I made of the histories of twins who were closely alike, it was most commonly said that one of the twins was guided by the other. I suppose that after their many childish struggles for supremacy, each finally discovered his own relative strength of character, and thenceforth the stronger developed into the leader, while the weaker contentedly subsided into the position of being led. Again, it is sometimes observed that one member of an otherwise easygoing family, discovers that he or she may exer- cise considerable power by adopting the habit of being persistently disagreeable whenever he or she does not get the first and best of everything. Some wives contrive to tyrannise over husbands who are mild and sensitive, who hate family scenes and dread the dis- grace attending them, by holding themselves in readiness to fly into a passion whenever their wishes are withstood. They thus acquire a habit of " breaking out," to use a term familiar to the warders of female prisons and lunatic asylums ; and though their relatives and connections would describe their tempers by severe epithets, yet if they had married masterful husbands their characters might have developed more favourably. To recapitulate briefly, one set of influences tends to mix good and bad tempers in a family at haphazard ; another set tends to assimilate them, so that they shall all be good or all be bad ; a third set tends to divide each family into contrasted portions. We have now to ascertain the facts and learn the results of these opposing influences. In dealing with the distribution of temper in Fraternities,^ we ^ A Fraternity consists of the brothers of a family, and of the sisters after the qualities of the latter have been transmuted to their Male Equivalents ; but as no change in the Female values seems really needed, so none has been made in respect to Temper. APPENDIX D. 235 can only make use of those in which at least two cases of temper are recorded ; they are 146 in number. I have removed all the cases of neutral temper, treating them as if they were non-existent, and dealing only with the remainder that are good or bad. We have next to eliminate the haphazard element. Beginning with Fraternities of two persons- only, either of whom is just as likely to be good as bad tempered, there are, as we have already seen, four possible combinations, resulting in the proportions of 1 case of both good, 2 cases one good and one bad, and one case of both bad. I have 42 such Fraternities, and the observed facts are that in 10 of them both are good tempered, in 20 one is good and one bad, and in 12 both are bad tempered. Here only a trifling and un- trustworthy difference is found between the observed and the haphazard distribution, the other conditions having neutralised each other. But when we proceed to larger Fraternities the test becomes shrewder, and the trifling difference already observed becomes more marked, and is at length unmistakable. Thus the successive lines of Table III. show a continually increasing diverg- ence between the observed and the haphazard distribution of temper, as the Fraternities increase in size. A compendious com- TABLE 3. DlSTEIBTJTION OF TeMPEE IN FeATERNITIES. A. — Observed. B. — Haphazard. Number in each Fraternity. Numl>er of Fra- ternities. All good- tem- pers. Intermediate cases. All bad- tem- " pers. All good- tem- pers. Intermediate cases. All bad- tem- pers. 2 3 4 5 6 42 55 29 6 14 10 11 5 1 1 20 15 21 6 9 8 0 2 10 0 13 3 2 12 8 1 2 4 10 7 2 0 0 21 20 21 8 12 8 12 2 1 2 4 5 4 2 11 7 2 0 0 4 to 6 49 7 7 2 2 236 NATUR.VL INHERITANCE. parison is made in the bottom line of the Table by adding together the instances in which the Fraternities are from 4 to 6 in number, and in taking only those in which all the members of the Fraternity were alike in temper, whether good or bad. There are 7 + 7, or 14, observed cases of this against 2 + 2, or 4, haphazard cases, found in a total of 49 Fraternities. Hence it follows that the domestic influ- ences that tend to differentiate temper wholly fail to overcome the influences, hereditary and other, that tend to make it uniform in the same Fraternity. As regards direct evidences of heredity of temper, we must frame our inquiries under a just sense of the sort of materials we have to depend upon. They are but coarse portraits scored with white or black, and sorted into two heaps, irrespective of the gradations of tint in the originals. The processes I have used in discussing the heredity of stature, eye-colour, and artistic faculty, cannot be employed in dealing with the heredity of temper. I must now renounce those refined operations and set to work with ruder tools on my rough material. The first inquiry will be. Do good-tempered parents have, on the whole, good-tempered children, and do bad-tempered parents have bad-tempered ones? I have 43 cases where both parents are recorded as good-tempered, and 25 where they were both bad- tempered. Out of the children of the former, 30 per cent, were good-tempered and 10 per cent, bad; out of the latter, 4 per cent, were good and 52 per cent, bad-tempered. This is emphatic testi- mony to the heredity of temper, I have worked out the other less contrasted combinations of parental temper, but the results are hardly worth giving. There is also- much variability in the proportions of the neutral cases. I then attempted, with still more success, to answer the converse question. Do good-tempered Fraternities have, on the whole, good- tempered ancestors, and bad tempered Fraternities bad-tempered ones 1 After some consideration of the materials, I defined — rightly or wrongly — a good-tempered Fraternity as one in which at le itt two members were good-tempered and none were bad, and a bad- tempered Fraternity as one in which at least two members were bad-tempered, whether or no any cases of good temper were said to be associated with them. Then, as regards the ancestors, I thought APPENDIX D. 237 by far the most trustworthy group was that which consisted of the two parents and of the uncles and aunts on both sides. I have thus 46 good-tempered Fraternities with an aggregate of 333 parents, uncles, and aunts ; and 71 bad-tempered, with 638 parents, uncles, and aunts. In the former group, 26 per cent, were good tempered and 18 bad ; in the latter group, 18 were good-tempered and 29 were bad, the remainder being neutral. These results are almost the exact counterparts of one another, so I seem to have made good hits in framing the definitions. More briefly, wo mav say that when the Fraternity is good-tempered as above defined, the number of good-tempered parents, uncles, and aunts, exceeds that of the bad-tempered in the proportion of 3 to 2 ; and that when the Fraternity is bad-tempered, the proportions are exactly reversed. I have attempted in other ways to work out the statistics of hereditary tempers, but none proved to be of sufficient value for publication. I can trace no prepotency of one sex over the other in transmitting their tempers to their children. I find clear indications of strains of bad temper clinging to families for three generations, but I cannot succeed in putting them into a numerical form. It must not be thought that I have wished to deal with temper as if it were an unchangeable characteristic, or to assign more trustworthiness to my material than it deserves. Both these views have been discussed ; they are again alluded to to show that they are not dismissed from my mind, and partly to give the opportunity of adding a very few further remarks. Persons highly respected for social and public qualities may be well-known to their relatives as having sharp tempers under strong but insecure control, so that they " flare up " now and then. I have heard the remark that those who are over- suave in ordinary demeanour have often vile tempers. If this be the case — and I have some evidence of its truth — I suppose they are painfully conscious of their infirmity, and through habitual endeavours to subdue it, have insensibly acquired an exaggerated suavity at the times when their temper is unprovoked. Illness, too, has much influence in affecting the temper. Thus I sometimes come across entries to the effect of, " not naturally ill-tempered, but peevish 238 NATURAL INHERITANCE. through illness." Overwork and worry will make even mild-tempered men exceedingly touchy and cross. The accurate discernment and designation of character is almost beyond the reach of any one, but, on the other hand, a rough estimate and a fair description of its prominent features is easily obtainable ; and it seems to me that the testimony of a member of a family who has seen and observed a person in his unguarded moments and under very varied circumstances for many years, is a verdict deserving of much confidence. I shall have fulfilled my object in writing this paper if it leaves a clear impression of the great range and variety of temper among persons of both sexes in the upper and middle classes of English society ; of its disregard in Marriage Selection; of the great admixture of its good and bad varieties in the same family ; and of its being, nevertheless, as hereditary as any other quality. Also, that although it exerts an immense influence for good or ill on domestic happiness, it seems that good temper has not been especially looked for, nor ill temper especially shunned, as it ought to be in marriage- selection. E. THE GEOMETRIC MEAN, IN VITAL AND SOCIAL STATISTICS.^ My purpose is to show that an assumption which lies at the basis of the well-known law of " Frequency of Error" is incorrect when applied to many groups of vital and social phenomena, although that law has been applied to them by statisticians with partial success. Next, I will point out the correct hypothesis upon which a Law of Error suitable to these cases ought to be calculated ; and subsequently I will communicate a memoir by Mr. (now Dr.) Donald Macalister, who, at my suggestion, has mathematically investigated the subject. The assumption to which I refer is, that errors in excess or in deficiency of the truth are equally probable ; or conversely, that if two fallible measurements have been made of the same object, their ^ Epprinted, with slight revision, from the Proceedings of the Eoyal Society, No. 198, 1879. ^ APPENDIX E. 239 arithmetical mean is more likely to be the true measurement than any other quantity that can be named. This assumption cannot be justified in vital phenomena. For ex- ample, suppose we endeavour to match a tint ; Weber's law, in its approximative and simplest form, of Sensation varying as the logarithm of the Stimulus, tells us that a series of tints, in which the quantities of white scattered on a black ground are as 1, 2, 4, 8, 16, 32, &c., will appear to the eye to be separated by equal in- tervals of tint. Therefore, in matching a grey that contains 8 por- tions of white, we are just as likely to err by selecting one that has 16 portions as one that has 4 portions. In the first case there Would be an error in excess, of 8 units of absolute tint ; in the second there would be an error in deficiency, of 4. Therefore, an error of the same magnitude in excess or in deficiency is not equally probable in the judgment of tints by the eye. Conversely, if two persons, who are equally good judges, describe their impressions of a certain tint, and one says that it contains 4 portions of white and the other that it contains 16 poitions, the most reasonable conclu- sion is that it really contains 8 portions. The arithmetic mean of the two estimates is 10, which is not the most probable value ; it is the geometric mean 8, (4 : 8 : : 8 : 16), which is the most probable. Precisely the same condition characterises every determination by each of the senses; for exa^mple, in judgiug of the weight of bodies or of their temperatures, of the loudness and of the pitches of tones, and of estimates of lengths and distances as wholes. Thus, three rods of the lengths a, b, c, when taken successiv^ely in the hand, appear to dilf er by equal intervals when a : h : : b : c, and not when a—b^b - G. In all physiological phenomena, where there is on the one hand a stimulus and on the other a response to that stimulus Weber's or some other geometric law may be assumed to prevail in other words, the true mean is geometric rather than arithmetic. The geometric mean appears to be equally applicable to the ma- jority of the influences, which, combined with those of purely vital phenomena, give rise to the events with which sociology deals. It is difficult to find terms sufficiently general to apply to the varied topics of sociology, but there are two categories which are of common oc- currence in which the geometric mean is certainly appropriate. The one is increase, as exemplified by the growth of population, where an 240 NATURAL INHERITANCE. already large nation tends to receive larger accessions than a small oDe under similar circumstances, or when a capital employed in a business increases in proportion to its size. The other category is the influences of circumstances or of " milieux " as they are often called, such as a period of plenty in which a larger held or a larger business yields a greater excess over its mean yield than a smaller one. Most of the causes of those differences with which sociology are concerned, and which are not purely vital phenomena, such as those previously discussed, may be classified under one or other of these two categories, or under such as are in principle almost the same. In short, sociological phenomena, like vital phenomena are, as a general rule, subject to the condition of the geometric mean. The ordinary law of Frequency of Error, based on the arithmetic mean, corresponds, no doubt, sufficiently well with the observed facts of vital and social phenomena, to be very serviceable to statisticians, but it is far from satisfying their wants, and it may lead to absurdity when applied to wide deviations. It asserts that deviations in excess must be balanced by deviations of equal magnitude in deficiency ; therefore, if the former be greater than the mean itself, the latter must be less than zero, that is, must be negative. This is an impossi- bility in many cases, to which the law is nevertheless applied by sta- tisticians with no small success, so long as they are content to confine its application within a narrow range of deviation. Thus, in respect of Stature, the law is very correct in respect to ordinary measure- ments, although it asserts that the existence of giants, whose height is more than double the mean height of their race, implies the possi- bility of the existence of dwarfs, whose stature is less than nothing at all. It is therefore an object not only of theoretical interest but of practical use, to thoroughly investigate a Law of Error, based on the geometric mean, even though some of the expected results may perhaps be apparent at first sight. With this view I placed the fore- going remarks in Mr. Donald Macalister's hands, who contributed a memoir that will be found in the Froc. Royal Soc, No. 198, 1879, following my own. It should be referred to by such mathematicians as may read this book. APPENDIX F. 241 F. PROBABLE EXTINCTION OF FAMILIES.^ The decay of the families of men who occupied conspicuous posi- tions in past times has been a subject of frequent remark, and has given rise to various conjectures. It is not only the families of men of genius or those of- the aristocracy who tend to perish, but it is those of all with whom history deals, in any way, even such men as the burgesses of towns, concerning whom Mr. Doubleday has inquired and written. The instances are very numerous in which surnames that were once common have since become scarce or have wholly disappeared. The tendency is universal, and, in explanation of it, the conclusion has been hastily drawn that a rise in physical comfort and intellectual capacity is necessarily accompanied by diminution in "fertility" — using that phrase in its widest sense and reckoning abstinence from marriage as one cause of sterility. If that conclusion be true, our population is chiefly maintained through the " proletariat," and thus a large element of degradation is inseparably connected with those other elements which tend to ameliorate the race. On the other hand, M. Alphonse de Candolle has directed attention to the fact that, by the ordinary law of chances," a large proportion of families are continually dying out, and it evidently follows that, until we know what that proportion is, we cannot estimate whether any observed diminution of sur- names among the families whose history we can trace, is or is not a sign of their diminished "fertility." I give extracts from M. De Candolle' s work in a foot-note,^ and may add that, although I have not hitherto published anything on the matter, I took considerable pains some years ago to obtain numerical results in respect to this 1 Reprinted, with slight revision, from the Journ. Anthropol. Inst. 1888. 2 " An milieu des renseignements precis et des opinions tres-sensees de MM. Benoiston de Chateaunenf, Galton, et autres statisticiens, je n'ai pas ren- contre la reflexion bien importaiite qu'ils auraient du faire de I'extinction inevitable des nonis de famille. Evidemment tons les noms doivent s'eteindre .... Un matliematicien pourrait calculer comment la reduction des noms ou titres aurait lieu, d'apres la probabilite des naissances toutes feminines ou toutes masculines ou melangees et la probabilite d'absence de naissances dans un couple quelconque," &c. — Alphoxse de Candolle, Histoire des Sciences et des Savants, 1873. R 242 NATURAL INHERITANCE. very problem. I made certain very simple and not very inaccurate suppositions concerning average fertility, and I worked to the nearest integer, starting with 10,000 persons, but the computation became intolerably tedious after a few steps, and I had to abandon it. The Rev. H. W. Watson kindly, at my request, took the pro- blem in hand, and his results form the subject of the following paper. They do not give what can properly be called a general solution, but they do give certain general results. They show (1) how to compute, though with great labour, any special case ; (2) a remarkably easy way of computing those special cases in which the law of fertility approximates to a certain specified form; and (3) how all surnames tend to disappear. The form in which I originally stated the problem is as follows. I purposely limited it in the hope that its solution might be more practicable if unnecessary generalities were excluded : — A large nation, of whom we will only concern ourselves with the adult males, N in number, and who each bear separate surnames, colonise a district. Their law of population is such that, in each generation, a^ per cent, of the adult males have no male children who reach adult life ; a^ have one such male child ; a^ have two ; and so on up to a^y who have five. Find (1) what proportion of the surnames will have become extinct after r generations ; and (2) how many instances there will be of the same surname being held by m persons. Discussion of the iwobhm hy the Rev. H. W. Watson, D.Sc, F.R.S., formerly Fellow of Trinity College, Gamhridge. Suppose that at any instant all the adult males of a large nation have different surnames, it is required to find how many of these surnames will have disappeared in a given number of genera- tions upon any hypothesis, to be determined by statistical investiga- tions, of the law of male population. Let, therefore, a^ be the percentage of males in any generation who have no sons reaching adult life, let a^ be the percentage that have one such son, «2 the percentage that have two, and so on up to a^, the percentage that have q such sons, q being so large that it is not worth while to consider the chance of any man having more than q adult sons — our first hypothesis will be that the numbers APPENDIX F. 243 a^, a-^, ^2' 6^^-? remain the same in each succeeding generation. We shall also, in what follows, neglect the overlapping of genera- tions— that is to say, we shall treat the problem as if all the sons born to any man in any generation came into being at one birth, and as if every man's sons were born and died at the same time. Of course it cannot be asserted that these assumptions are correct. Yery probably accurate statistics would discover variations in the values of CTq, a-^, etc., as the nation progressed or retrograded ; but it is not at all likely that this variation is so rapid as seriously to vitiate any general conclusions arrived at on the assumption of the values remaining the same through many successive generations. It is obvious also that the generations must overlap, and the neglect to take account of this fact is equivalent to saying, that at any given time we leave out of consideration those male descendants, of any original ancestor who are more than a certain average number of generations removed from him, and compensate for this by giving credit for such male descendants, not yet come into being, as are not more than that same average number of generations removed from the original ancestors. Let then -^, —J-, —-4-, etc., up to -—^ be denoted by the sym- 100 100 100 ^100 ^ ^ bols i^Q, t^, t^j etc., up to ^g, in other words, let ^q, t-^, etc., be the chances in the first and each succeeding generation of any individual man, in any generation, having no son, one son, two sons, and so on, who reach adult life. Let N be the original number of distinct sur- names, and let ^m^ be the fraction of IST which indicates the number of such surnames with s representatives in the rth generation. Now, if any surname have p representatives in any generation, it follows from the ordinary theory of chances that the chance of that same surname having s representatives in the next succeeding gene- ration is the coefficient of x^ in the expansion of the multinomial (^0 -1- t-^x 4- t^x^ -i- , etc. 4- t^x'^y Let then the expression t^ + t-^x -f t^x'^ -f etc. -f t^x*^ be repre- sented by the symbol T. Then since, by the assumption already made, the number of sur- names with no representative in the r-lth generation is ^.^m^ N, the K 2 244 NATURAL INHERITANCE. number with one representative ^_;^m^.N, the number with two r.im.2. N and so on, it follows, from what we last stated, that the number of surnames with s representatives in the rth generation must be the coefficient of x' in the expression I ^.imo + ,_imiT + ,.im2T2+ etc. +,_iW,,_iT^'-i |n If, therefore, the coefficient of N in this expression be denoted by y^ (x) it follows that r-i^n-^^, r-i^h ^^^ ^^ ^^' ^^® ^^^ coefficients of x, x^ and so on, in the expression y]._j^ {x). If, therefore, a series of functions be found such that f^{x):=^tQ + t-^x+ etc. +tqX'^ and/]. {x)=/,._-^ (f^o + ^i^ ®*^- +^^^*) then the proportional number of groups of surnames with s represen- tatives in the rth generation will be the coefficient of x^inf^ {x) and the actual number of such surnames will be found by multiplying this coefficient by N. The number of surnames nonrepresented or be- come extinct in the rth generation will be found by multiplying the term independent of x in./^ (x) by the number N. The determination, therefore, of the rapidity of extinction of sur- names, when the statistical data, t^, t-^, etc., are given, is reduced to the mechanical, but generally laborious process of successive substi- tution of Iq + t-^x 4- t.^x^ + etc., for x in successively determined values of y,. (x), and no further progress can be made with the problem until these statistical data are fixed ; the following illustrations of the ap- plication of our formula are, however, not without interest. (1) The very simplest case by which the formula can be illustrated is when q = 2 and t^, tj, t^ are each equal to J. Here f, (x) = l±|±^/,(x) = I j 1 + ^ {l+x + x'^)+ \ l+x + x^)^J and so on. Making the successive substitutions, we obtain _j. , . 1 ( 13 5a3 Qx^ 2x X \ f-, {x)=.— { 1 \-— -\ 1-— 1- ''"^ ^ 3(99999/ ■^''^ ^ 2187 2187 2187 2187 2187 2187 2187 2187*^ 2l87 fpi)= -63183+ •08306a; + -10635x2+ •07804a;3+ ■0Qi89x^+ -05443^5+ .oi437a;6 + -01692:^7+ •01144a;8+ -00367x9+ -00104x10+ '00015x11+ -00005x^2 + -00001x13 + -00000x1* + -00000x15 + -OOOOOx^^ APPENDIX F. 245 and the constant term in./^ (x) or ^ttIq is therefore «.oiQQ . '08306 , -10635 , '07804 , '06489 , '05443 , '01437 , '01692 , '01144 0 oioo + + + + + + + + 3 9 27 81 243 729 2187 6561 •00367 '00104-00015 19683 59049 177147 The value of which to five places of decimals is '67528. The constant terms, therefore, in y^, /^ up to /^ when reduced to decimals, are in this case '33333, '48148, '57110., '64113, and -65628 respectively. That is to say, out of a million surnames at starting, there have disappeared in the course of one, two, etc., up to five generations, 333333, 481480, 571100, 641130, and 675280 re- spectively. The disappearances are much more rapid in the earlier than in the later generations. Three hundred thousand disappear in the first generation, one hundred and fifty thousand more in the second, and so on, while in passing from the fourth to the fifth, not more than thirty thousand surnames disappear. All this time the male population remains constant. For it is evident that the male population of any generation is to be found by multiplying that of the preceding generation, by ^^ + 2^2? ^^^ ^^^^ quantity is in the present case equal to one. If axes Ox and Ot/ be drawn, and equal distances along Ox repre- sent generations from starting, while two distances are marked along every ordinate, the one representing the total male population in any generation, and the other the number of remaining surnames in that generation, of the two curves passing through the extremities of these ordinates, the j^opulation curve w^ll, in this case, be a straight line parallel to Ox, while the surname curve will intersect the popu- lation curve on the axis of y, will proceed always convex to the axis of X, and will have the positive part of that axis for an asymptote. The case just discussed illustrates the use to be made of the general formula, as well as the labour of successive substitutions, when the expressions/^ (x) does not follow some assigned law. The calcula- tion may be infinitely simplified when such a law can be found ; especially if that law be the expansion of a binomial, and only the extinctions are required. For example, suppose that the terms of the expression tQ + t-^x + etc. + tgCCg are proportional to the terms of the expanded binomial 246 NATURAL INHERITANCE. (a-\-hxY i.e. suppose tliat fr. — „ t\=^Q ;— and so on. Here/, (^)=^ — -,, and .m. = . — '^^ ^ ^ {a + by ^ ^ {a + bf J- / \ 1 / , i{a + bxy\q f^ (^) ^ 1 7T 1 « + 0^-7 TV- \ 2^/^o "^ " ^ "^ ^ {a + bY\ ' M Generally .m, = ^-1^^ { « + 5.,mo } ^ = ^^^ { ^ + ..,m, } ^ If, therefore, we wish to find the number of extinctions in any generation, we have only to take the number in the preceding generation, add it to the constant fraction _ , raise the sum to the b power of q, and multiply by {a-\-bY "With the aid of a table of logarithms, all this may be effected for a great number of generations in a very few minutes. It is by no means unlikely that when the true statistical data ifg, t^^, etc., t^ are ascertained, values of a, b, and q may be found, which shall render the terms of the expansion (a + bxy approximately proportionate to the terms of/ (x). If this can be done, we may ajjproximate to the determination of the rapidity of extinction with very great ease, for any number of generations, however great. For example, it does not seem very unlikely that the value of q might be 5, while t^, t^...t^ might be '237, '396, 264, -088, '014, -001, or nearly, J, J, -^\, -^j, ^-i^, and jJ^o- (3 + xY 3^ Should that be the case, we have, /j (x) = -^ ^ ^rriQ = _ and generally ^m^ = 75 ] 3 + ^.-^ttiq > 45 Thus we easily get for the number of extinctions in the first ten generations respectively. •237, -346, -410, -450, -477, -496, -510, -520, -527, -533. We observe the same law noticed above in the case of — 3 viz., that while 237 names out of a thousand disappear in the first APPENDIX F. 247 step, and an additional 109 names in the second step, there are only 27 disappearances in the fifth step, and only six disappearances in the tenth step. If the curves of surnames and of population were drawn from this case, the former would resemble the corresponding curve in the case last mentioned, while the latter would be a curve whose distance from the axis of x increased indefinitely, inasmuch as the expression ^1 + 2^2 +^^3 + 4^4 + ^^5 is greater than one. "Whenever /-^ (x) can be represented by a binomial, as above sug- gested, we get the equation whence it follows that as r increases indefinitely the value of ^m^ ap- proaches indefinitely to the value ?/ where ^=(^){''+^4 that is where y=l. All the surnames, therefore, tend to extinction in an indefinite time, and this result might have been anticipated generally, for a sur- name once lost can never be recovered, and there is an additional chance of loss in every successive generation. This result must not be confounded with that of the extinction of the male population ; for in every binomial case where q is greater than 2, we have t-^ + 21^ + etc. -\-qt^>l, and, therefore an indefinite increase of male population. The true interpretation is that each of the quantities, ^m^, Ml 2' etc., tends to become zero, as t is indefinitely increased, but that it does not follow that the product of each by the infinitely large num- ber N is also zero. As, therefore, time proceeds indefinitely, the number of surnames extinguished becomes a number of the same order of magnitude as the total number at first starting in N, while the number of surnames represented by one, two, three, etc., lepresentatives is some infinitely smaller but finite number. "When the finite numbers are multiplied by the corresponding number of representatives, sometimes infinite in number, and the products added together, the sum will generally ex- ceed the original number N. In point of fact, just as in the cases calculated above to five generations, we had a continual, and indeed 248 NATURAL INHERITANCE. at first, a rapid extinction of surnames, combined in the one case with a stationary, and in the other case an increasing population, so is it when the number of generations is increased indefinitely. We have a continual extinction of surnames going on, combined with constancy, or increase of population, as the case may be, until at length the number of surnames remaining is absolutely insensible, as compared with the number at starting ; but the total number of representatives of those remaining surnames is infinitely greater than the original number. We are not in a position to assert from actual calculation that a corresponding result is true for every form oif^ (x), but the reason- able inference is that such is the case, seeing that it holds whenever /j (x) may be compared with ^ y whatever a, h, or q may be. G. ORDERLY ARRANGEMENT OF HEREDITARY DATA. There are many methods both of drawing pedigrees and of describing kinship, but for my own purposes I still prefer those that I designed myself. The chief requirements that have to be fulfilled are compactness, an orderly and natural arrangement, and clearly intelligible symbols. Nomenclature.— A. symbol ought to be suggestive, consequently the initial letter of a word is commonly used for the purpose. But this practice would lead to singular complications in symbolizing the various ranks of kinship, and it must be applied sparingly. The letter F is equally likely to suggest any one of the three very diffe- rent words of Father, Female, and Fraternal. The letter M suggests both Mother and Male ; S would do equally for Son and for Sister. Whether they are English, French, or German words, much the same complexity prevails. The system employed in Hereditary Genius had the merit of brevity, but was apt to cause mistake ; it was awkward in manuscript and difficult to the printer, and I have now abandoned it in favour of the method employed in the Records APPENDIX G. 249 of Family Faculties, This will now be briefly described again. Each kinsman can be described in two ways, either by letters or by a number. In ordinary cases both the letter and number are intended to be used simultaneously, thus FF.8. means the Father's Father of the person described, though either FF or 8, standing by themselves, would have the same meaning. The double nomen- clature has great practical advantages. It is a check against mis- take and makes reference and orderly arrangement easy. As regards the letters, F stands for Father and M for Mother, whenever no letter succeeds them ; otherwise they stand for Father's and for Mother's respectively. Thus F is Father ; FM is Father's Mother ; FMF is Father's Mother's Father. As regards the principle upon which the numbers are assigned, arithmeticians will understand me when I say that it is in accord- ance with the binary system of notation, which runs parallel to the binary distribution of the successive ranks of ancestry, as two parents, four grandparents, eight great-grandparents, and so on. The '' subject " of the pedigree is of generation O ; that of his parents, of generation 1'; that of his grandparents, of generation 2, &c. This is clearly shown in the following table : — Kinship. Order of Genera- tion. Numerical Yalues in Binary Notation. in Decimal Nota- tion. Child 0 1 1 Parents 1 10 11 2 . 3 Gr. Par. 2 100 101 110 111 4 5 6 7 G. Gr. Par. ...... 3 < 1000 1001 1010 1011 1100 1100 1110 1111 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 All the male ancestry are thus described by even numbers and the female ancestry by odd ones. They run as follows : — 250 NATURAL INHERITANCE. E,2. M, 3. FF, 4. FM, 5. MF, 6. MM, 7. FFF, 8. FMF, 10. MFF, 12. MMF, 14. FFM, 9. FMM, 11. MFM, 13. MMM, 15. It will be observed that the double of the number of any ancestor is that of his or her Father ; and that the double of the number 2olus 1 is that of his or her Mother ; thus FM 5 has for her father FMF 10, and for her mother FMM 11. When the word Brother or Sister has to be abbreviated it is safer not to be too stingy in assigning letters, but to write hr, sr, and in the plural hrs, srs ; also for the long phrase of '' brothers and sisters," to write hrss. All these symbols are brief enough to save a great deal of space, and they are perfectly explicit. When such a phrase has to be expressed as " the Fraternity of whom FF is one " I write in my own notes simply FF', but there has been no occasion to adopt this symbol in the present book. I have not satisfied myself as to any system for expressing descendants. Theoretically, the above binary system admits of extension by the use of negative indices, but the practical applica- tion of the idea seems cumbrous. We and the French sadly want a word that the Germans possess to stand for Brothers and Sisters. Fraternity refers properly to the brothers only, but its use has been legitimately extended here to mean the brothers and the sisters, after the qualities of the latter have been reduced to their male equivalents. The Greek word adelpliic would do for an adjective. Pedigrees. — The method employed in the Record of Family Faculties for entering all the facts concerning each kinsman in a methodical manner was fully described in that book, and could not easily be epitomised here ; but a description of the method in which the manuscript extracts from the records have been made for my own use will be of service to others when epitomising their own family characteristics. It will be sufficient to describe the quarto books that contain the medical extracts. Each page is ten and a half inches high and eight and a half wide, and the two pages, 252, 253, that are APPENDIX G. 251 P H o ^1 fH (B (D ri:1 rCj 03 03 -)J -(J ?-( O 1^ &1 !=^ 03 +J 03 g ^"'3 1 1 a^H C/2 JO l9 S O^ CO f^ ^ .2 02 i^ %% fe-r) In '^ 03 M -^ 9 o 13 rt o i' o o W % % , 's . 03 4S 03 Pm rC3 CS -1^ pH ■5 t3 O ^ ^ 03 3 u r-l (13 i-Sh r^ 03 a5 -M • -M , 53 '^ 03 if o 53 O «*-! _aj ciU ^ CO l=H .1-1 03 3 M --^ ■ ^-^ -1-^ "—I "h 'Ti "ph 't:! 03 rrr*, M 03 Pi o o C^ ^ c3 °3 U 03 03 O l-H 03 +? Ph 03 03 >-5 CO !>2 03 ^ ^ 03 P-l 03 (T) CO -fH 03 M w^ .«=^ In'^ "Vh "TIJ 5^ 03 g 03* , 2 i=l ^ 03 P^ p^ C« C3 CO M P^ E=4 252 NATURAL INHERITANCE. Example A. Father's name James Gladding. Mother's maiden name .... Maey Claeemont. Initials. J. G. R. G- W. G. F. L. C. G. M. G. A. C. W. C. E. C. F. R. R.N. L. C. Kin. Principal illnesses and cause of death. Age at death. Father Bad rheum, fever ; agonising headaches ; diar- rhoea ; bronchitis ; pleurisy . . Heart disease 54 56 83 73 63 46 62 19 85 50 21 bro. bro. sis. sis. Rheum, gout Apoplexy Good health except gout ; paralysis later Apoplexy Rheum, fever and rheum, gout . . . Apoplexy Delicate (inoculated) Small pox Mother Tendency to lung disease ; biliousness ; fre- quent heart attacks . Heart disease and dropsy bro. bro. bro. sis. sis. sis. Good health . Accident Led a wild life Premature old age Always delicate Consumption Small-pox three times .... General failure Bilious ; weak health Cancer Fever M. G. K. G. G. L. F. S. R. F. L. G. bro. bro. sis. sis. sis. sis. Inflam. lungs ; rheum, fever . . Heart disease Debility ; heart disease ; colds . Consumption Bai headaches ; coughs ; weak spine ; hysteria ; apoplexy Paralysis Bad colds ; inflam. lungs ; hysteria Infantile paralysis ; colds ; nervous depression . Inflam. brain, also lungs ; neuralgia ; nervous fever 17 40 50 living living living (Space left for remarks.) APPENDIX G. 253 Example B. Fatlier's name Julius Fitzeoy. Mother's maiden name . . . Amelia Mereyweathee. Initials. R. F. L. F.i A. G. F. W. F. P. M. A. M. N. M. R. B. C. M. F. L. Kin. Father bro. hro. bro. Mother bro. bro. bro, sis. • sis. sis. Principal illnesses and cause of death. Gouty Habit . . Weak Heart and Congest. Liver Gout and Decay . . . . Accideiit . . . . Typhoid Gall stones Internal Malady (?) Cancer Still living. Paralysis Paralysis 1 died an infant. . . Consumption PJieum. in Head Softening of Brain Age at death. 73 48 16 55 86 85 33 88 76 G. F. H. F. S. T. F. H. G. H. B.R. N. F. E. L. F. bro. bro. bro. sis. sis. sis. sis. Gout : tendency to mesenteric disease ; eruptive disorders . . , Blood poisoning after a cut Liver deranged ; bad headaches ; once supposed consumptive Gradual Paralysis Eruptive disorder ; mesentery disease ; inflam- mation of liver . . Inflamrtiation of Lungs Eruptive disorder ; liver . . . Infiam. of Lungs Delicate ; tend, to mesent. disease , Consumption Colds ; liver disorder Consumpition Mesenteric disease ; grandular swellings . Atrophy 2 died infants. 46 45 42 47 29 30 16 (Space left for remarks.) 254 NATURAL INHERITANCE. found wherever the book is opened, relate to the same family. The open book is ruled so as to resemble the accompanying schedule, which is drawn on a reduced scale on page 251. The printing within the compartments of the schedule does not appear in the MS. books, it is inserted here merely to show to whom each compartment refers. It will be seen that the paternal ancestry are described in the left page, the maternal in the right. The method of arrangement is quite orderly, but not altogether uniform. To avoid an unpleasing arrangement like a tree with branches, and which is very wasteful of space, each grandparent and his own two parents are arranged in a set of three compartments one above the other. There are, of course, four grandparents and therefore four such sets in the schedule. Reference to the examples A andB pages 252 and 253 will show how these compartments are filled up. The rest of the Schedule explains itself. The children of the pedigree are written below the compartment assigned to the mother and her brothers and sisters ; the spare spaces are of much occasional service, to receive the overfi.ow from some of the already filled compartments as well as for notes. It is astonishing how much can be got into such a schedule by writing on ruled paper with the lines one-sixth of an inch apart, which is not too close for use. Of course the writing must be small, but it may be bold, and the figures should be written very distinctly. For a less ambitious attempt, including the grandparents and their fraternity, but not going further back, the left-hand page would sufiice, placing " Children " where " Father " now stands, "Father's Father" for "Father," and so on throughout. INDEX. INDEX. Accidents, 19, 55, 65 Acquired peculiarities, inheritance of, 14 Album for Life History, 220 Ancestral contributions, 134 Adelphic, 250 Aiitliropometric Committee of the British Assoc, 95 ; anthrop. labo- ratory data, 43, 46, 79 Apparatus, see Models Acquired faculties, transmission of, 16, 197 Artistic Faculty, 154 Averages tell little, 36 ; comparison of with medians, 41 Awards for E.F.F., 75 Bartholomew's Hospital, 47 Bias, Statistical effect of, in marriage selection, 162 ; in suppressing cause of death, 168 Blends, or refusals to blend, 12 ; in issue of unlike parents, 89 ; in hazel eyes, 145 ; in diseases, 169 ; in new varieties, 198 ; in temper, 233 ; have no effect on statistical results, 17 Cabs, 26, 30, 31 Candolle, A de, 142, 145, 241 n Cards, illustration by, 188 Chance, 19 Chaos, order in, 66 Child, its relation to mother, 15 ; to either parent, 19; of drunken mother, 15 ; of consumptive mother, 177 Cloud compared to a population, 164 Co-Fraternities, 94 Consumption, 171 ; consumptivity, 181 Contributions from separate ancestors, 134 Cookery, typical dishes, 24 Cousins, their nearness in kinship, 133 ; marriages between, 175 Crelle's tables, 7 Crowds, characteristic forms, 23 Curve of Frequency, 40, 49 ; of Dis- tribution, 40, 54 Darwin, 4, 19 Data, 71 De Candolle, 142, 145, 241 n Deviations, schemes of, 51, 60 ; cause of, 55. See Error. Dickson, J. D. H., 69, 102, 115, Ap- pendix B., 221 Disease, 164; skipping a generation, 12 Distribution, schemes of, 37 ; normal curve of, 54 Error, curve of Frequency, 49 ; law of, 55 ; probable, 57 Evolution not by minute steps only, 32 Eye coloue., 138 Families, extinction of, Ajipendix F., 241 Family Records, see Records Father, see Parent Files in a squadron, 111 Filial relation, 19 ; regression, 95 Fraternities, meaning of word, 94, 234 ; to be treated as units, 35 ; issued from unlike parents, 90 ; variability in, 124, 129 ; regression in, 108 S 258 INDEX. Frequency, Scheme of, 49 ; model, 63 surface of, 102 Museums to illustrate evolution, 33 Music, 155, 158 Geometric Mean, 118, Appendix E, 238 Gouffe, cookery book, 24 Governments, 22 Grades, 37, 40 ; of modulus, mean error, &c., 57 Grove battery, 31 Natural, its meaning, 4 ; natural selec- tion, 32, 119 Nephews, 133 Nile expedition, 23 Nomenclature of kinship, 248 NOEMAL YARI ABILITY, 51 Hazel eyes, 144 Hospital, St. Bartholomew's, 47 Humphreys, G., 186 Incomes of the Euglish, 35 Infertility of mixed types, 31 Insurance companies, 185 Inventions, 25 Island and islets, 10 Kenilworth Castle, 21 Kinship, formula of, 114 ; table of nearness in different degrees, 132 Laboratory, anthropometric, 43, 46, 79 Landscape, characteristic features, 23 Latent Elements, 187 ; characters, 11 M, its signification, 41 Macalister, D., 238 McKendrick, Professor, 20 Malformation, 176 Marriage selection, in temper, 85, 232 ; in eye-colour, 86, 147 ; in stature, 87 ; in artistic faculty, 157 Means, 41 ; mean error, 57 Mechanical inventions, 25 Median, 41 Medical students, 47 Merrifield, F., 136 Mid, 41 ; Mid-parent, 87 ; mid-popula- tion, 92 ; mid-fraternity, 124 ; mid- error, 58 Models, to illustrate Stability, 27 ; Curve of Frequency, 63 ; Forecast of stature of children, 107 ; Surface of Frequency, 115 Modulus, its grade, 57 Moths, pedigree, 136 Mother's relation to her child, 1 5 ; in consumption, 177. See Parent Mulatto blends, 13 O'Brien, 84 Omnibuses, 26, 32 Order in apparent chaos, Q'd OEaANic Stability, 4 Paget, Sir J., 47 Palmer, 48 Pangenesis, 19, 193 Parents, unlike in stature, issue of, 88 ; indirect relation to children, 19 ; parental relation, 110, 132 Particulate inheritance, 7 Peas, experiments with, 79, 225 Peculiarities, natural and acquired, 4 ; inheritance of, 138 ; definition, 138, 194 Pedigree moths, 136 Pedigrees, arrangement of, see Appen- dix G., 248, 250 Percentiles, 46 Personal elements, 187 ; in incipient structure, 9 Petty influences, 16 Population to be treated as units, 35 ; their mid-stature, 92 ; their Q, 93 : successive generations of, 115 ; com- pared to a cloud, 164 Priestley, Dr. W, O., 15 Probability integral, 54 ; tables, 202 to 205 Probable error, 53, 57 Problems in the law of error, 66 Processes in Heredity, 4 Pure Breed, 189 Q, its meaning, 53, 59 R. F.F., see Records of Family Faculties Rank of Faculty, 36, 46 ; in a squadron, 110 Records of Family Faculties, data, 72 ; arrangement of, 250 ; prefaced re- marks, 168 ; object of book, 220 ; trustworthiness of, 130, 167, 231 INDEX. 259 Regression, filial, 95, 98, 131 ; mid- parental, 99, 101 ; parental, 100 ; fraternal, 109 ; generally, 103, 110, 114 ; is a measure of nearness of kinship, 132 ; in artistic faculty, 158; in consuniptivity, 181 ; an element of stability in characteristics of a people, 163 Schemes of Distribution and Fre- quency, 35 Scientific societies, their government, 22 Selection in marriage, 85, 157, 147 ; eff"ect of bias in, 162 ; Natural Selec- tion, 32, 119 Ships of war, 123 Simplification of inquiries into heredity, 191 Solomon, 24 Special data, 78 Sports, stability of, 30, 198 Squadron, 110 Stable forms, 20, 123, 198 ; subordi- nate, 25 Stability of sports, 30, 234 ; of charac- teristics in a people, 163 Statistics, are processes of blending, 1 7 ; charms of, 62 Stature, discussion oe Data, 83 Strength, Scheme of, 37 Structure, incipient, 18 Sweet peas, 79, 225 Swiss guides, 23 Temper, Appendix D., 236, 85 Traits, 9 Transmutation of female measures, 5, 42, 78 Trustworthiness of the data, 130, 167, 231 Types, 24 ; a marked family type, 234 Uncles, 133, 191 Variability of Stature, in population, 93 ; in Mid-Parents, 93 ; in Co-Fra- ternities, 94 ; in Fraternities, 124 ; in a pure breed, 189 Yariation, individual, 9 Watson, Rev. H. W., 242 AVeissman, 193 THE END. RICHARD CLAY AND SDKS, LIMITED, LONDON AND BUNGAY, Works by the Same Author. METEOBOGEAPHICA; or, Methods of Mapping the Weather. Illustrated by upwards of 600 Printed and Lithographed Diagrams. 4to. 9s. ENGLISH MEN OP SCIENCE : Their Nature and Nurture. 8vo. 8s, Qd. INQUIRIES INTO HUMAN FAC- ULTY AND ITS DEVELOPMENT. With Illustrations and Coloured and Plain Plates. Demy Svo. 16s. RECOBD OP FAMILY FACULTIES. Consisting of Tabular Forms and Directions for Entering Data, with an Explanatory Preface. 4to. 2s. Qd. LIFE HISTORY ALBUM; Being a Personal Note-book, combining the chief advantages of a Diary, Photograph Album, a Register of Height, Weight, and other Anthropometrical Observations, and a Record of Illnesses. Containing Tabular Forms, Charts, and Explanations especially designed for popular use. Prepared by the direction of the Collective Investigation Committee of the British Medical Association, and Edited by FRANCIS GALTON, F.R.S., Chairman of the Life History Sub-Committee. 4to. 3^. 6d. Or, with Cards of Wools for Testino^ Colour Vision, 4s. Qd. MACMILLAN & CO., LONDON. MESSRS. MACMILLAN & CO.'S PUBLICATIONS. WORKS BY ALFRED RUSSEL WALLACE, F.R.S. DarTvinism. Being a Systematic Exposition of the Theory of Natural Selection with some of its applications. Crown 8vo. [Shortly. The Geographical Distribution of Animals^ -with a Study of the Relations of Living and Sxtinct Faunas as elucidating the past changes of the Earth's Surface. With Numerous Illustrations hy Zwecker, and Maps. 2 vols. Medium 8vo. 42s. Island Life; or, The Phenomena and Causes of Insular Faunas and Floras, including a revision and attempted solution of the Problem of Geological Climates. With Illustrations and Maps. Demy 8vo. IBs. The Malay Archipelago: The Land of the Orang Utan and the Bird of Paradise. A Narrative of Travel. With Studies of Man and Nature. With Maps and Illustrations. Ninth Edition. Crown Svo. 7s. 6d. Anthropology : An Introduction to the Study of Man and Civilisation. By E. B. TYLOR, F.R.S. With Illustrations. Crown Svo. 7s. 6d. Romantic Love and Personal Beauty : Their Development, Causal Relation, Historic and National Peculiarities. By HENRY T. FINCK. 2 vols. Crown Svo. 18s, WORKS BY SIR JOHN LUBBOCK, BART., M.P., F.R.S., D.C.L., LL.D. The Origin and Metamorphoses of Insects. With Illustrations. New Edition. Crown Svo. .3s. 6d. [Nature Series. On British Wild Fl0"wers considered in their Relation to Insects. With numerous Illustrations. New Edition. Crown Svo. 4s. 6d. [Nature Series. Flowers, Fruits, and Leaves. With Illustrations. Crown Svo. 4s. 6d [Nature Seriet. WORKS BY THOMAS HENRY HUXLEY, F.R.S. Lay Sermons, Addresses, and Reviews. Eighth Edition. 8vo. Is.M. Essays, selected from Lay Sermons, Addresses, and Reviews. Third Edition. Crown Svo. Is. Critiques and Addresses. Sa^o. 10s. Qd. Physiography. An Introduction to the Study of Nature. With Coloured Plates and Woodcuts. Twelfth Edition. Crown Svo. &s. American Addresses, -with a Lecture on the Study of Biology. 8vo-° 6s. Qd. Science and Culture, and other Bssays. Demy Svo. 10s. Gd. Lessons in Elementary Physiology. With numerous Illustrations. Twenty second Edition. Fcap. Svo. 4s. Qd. Questions, Is. Qd. Introductory Primer. ISmo. Is. [Science Primers. WORKS BY W. K. CLIFFORD, F.R.S. Lectures and Essays. Edited by LESLIE STEPHEN and F. POLLOCK, with an Introduction by F. POLLOCK. With Two Portraits. 2 vols. Svo, 25s. Also an abridged Popular Edition. Crown Svo, 8s. 6d. Seeing and Thinking. With Diagrams. Crown Svo, 3s. 6d. [Nature Series. WORKS BY J. NORMAK LOCKYER, F.R.S., &c. Contributions to Solar Physics. With Illustrations. Royal Svo, 31s. 6d. The Chemistry of the Sun. With Illustrations. Svo, 14s. Primer of Astronomy. Illustrated. New Edition. ISmo, Is. Elementary Lessons in Astronomy. With numerous Illustrations and Coloured Diagram. New Edition. ISmo, 5s. 6d. Questions, Is. 6d. Outlines of Physiography.— The Movements of the Earth, Crown Svo, Is. 6d. MACMILLAN & CO., LONDON. BOSTON COLLEGE 3 9031 01657434 5 G^^f. BOSTON COLLEGE LIBRARY UNIVERSITY HEIGHTS CHESTNUT HILL, MASS. Books may be kept for two weeks and may be renewed ^d: the same .J^^fifll unless re- served. '"Oi. the same ^JJ^dfC. M^ P aay is Tw^XVJ^^^ l"*'3'ay is charged for each book kept overtime. If you cannot find what you want, ask the Librarian who will be glad to help you. The borrower is responsible for books drawn on his card and for all fines accruing on the same. :K,i-::NTAKO'-!