DUKE

UNIVERSITY

LIBRARY

DUKE UNIVERSITY LIBRARY

M4NUSCRIPT THESES

Tlii. ▼oluBo as; be oonsnlted freely, bat the literery rights of the author must be respected. No passage may be copied or closely paraphrased

without the preTious written consent of the author. If the reader obtains assistance froa this woluae he aust gire credit in his own work.

This thesis by John Edvard Kello has been used by the

following persons, whose signatures attest their aooeptanee of the abowe revtriotions.

cA. library borrowing this thesis for use by one of its patrons should seoure the signature of the user^

RAUB

ADDRESS

.1. ifWWOW

xi>|4t .,4*. IT ,5f.4^

■..*;if .«w'6 *i4:;;«i^>ti'4?» -Mu ,

nJ tJ Xj*>f/ .-»#*<< ••« oXM is’^jswM ivIoIm

-■ sr“‘ ■■ -'VT'';'*- , v-i ,'--^«^"T, ■‘r'S

, ,' ., . .. .i»1'.'N^,

,T4#r tii > io r'5#4««|4 # *#1- «Hjfro«itf ^tv^4« ^«^f^fs"’'V?isud

iT*a

(

^ "i i tif t;- •■

'■/.■'v'Efv;.! I"'-

w,V5

PMtl

^MS-

l«t>v

1^' ■#’.

fi?-

f ??l)

^ X

^ a#

, it

.X- Cv

ijiii. vj,

*.¥T b'^c "*L.

L ' . > "’ *s,|^ ^ s

c 'j'^-'itli. , ,.

L- ^ -•'

't'i

F * !<'/ tS

^4l .Y

■Hi

”, ^■.,3?.,-i

'life

jv, '*; X{'r;:i

i>'v‘

Digitized by the Internet Archive in 2016

https://archive.org/detaiis/observationofbehOOkeil

Duke University Library

The me of this thesis is subject to the usual restrictions that govern the use of manuscript material. Reproduction or quotation of the text is permitted only upon written authorization from the author of the thesis and from the academic department by which it was accepted. Proper acknowledgment must be given in all printed references or quotations.

OBSERVATION OF THE BEHAVIOR OF RATS RUNNING

TO REWARD AND NONREWARD IN AN ALLEYWAY

by

John Edward Kello

Department of Psychology Duke University

Dissertation submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in the Department of Psychology in the Graduate School of Duke University

1973

0

■■

ABSTRACT

(Psychology- Experimental)

OBSERVATION OF THE BEHAVIOR OF RATS RUNNING TO REWARD AND NONREWARD IN AN ALLEYWAY

by

John Edward Kello

Department of Psychology Duke University

Date;

Approved:

An abstract of a dissertation submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in the Department of Psychology in the Graduate School of Duke University

1973

ABSTRACT

OBSERVATION OF THE BEHAVIOR OF RATS RUNNING TO REWARD AND NONREWARD IN AN ALLEYWAY

by

John Edward Kello

The first experiment sought specifically to determine whether vari- ations in rats' overall run time in the alleyway reflect variations in the vigor of a single response, running (Hullian S-R view), or variations in the frequency of running relative to other, competing, behaviors, and not in the vigor of running ("response-competition" view). More broadly, the goal was a detailed descriptive account of the alleyway behavior of individual rats under a variety of typical experimental conditions.

Six 23-hr. hungry rats were run for 75 trials in an alleyway to various amounts of reward, and the amount was shifted at several stages of training. Also, an environmental stimulus change was introduced dur- ing training. Six 23-hr. thirsty rats were run under comparable condi- tions. Overall run time was recorded and each trial was video-taped. Each run was then classified as a competing behavior trial (subject ceases forward progress after entering alleyway), an accessory behavior trial

iii

(subject shows some additional behavior while running forward, but no full competing behavior), or a direct run trial (only running occurs).

The primary results were: (1) the decrease in run time in acquisi-

tion for each subject was due mainly to an orderly decrease in the amount of competing and accessory behavior, but the vigor of running also in- creased; (2) large changes in run time with other manipulations (differ- ences in amount of reward in acquisition, shifts in amount of reward, extra stimulus, and proximity to reward) also reflected changes in amount of competing and accessory behavior, and smaller changes in the vigor of running; (3) the behavior of the thirsty rats was qualitatively the same as the behavior of the hungry rats, but was less affected by reward. The thirsty rats ran slower and showed more overall variability and more com- peting and accessory behavior than the hungry ones.

The main implication of these results is that the alleyway is a multiple-response, selective-learning situation in which running both in- creases in vigor and gradually replaces other behaviors.

The second experiment examined the structure of behavior in extinc- tion to determine whether the increase in run time with nonreward reflects a return of competing and accessory behavior in the absence of selection for running. Secondary questions, not critical to a selective-learning view of the alleyway, were: (1) Would the addition of extra stimiili in

acquisition which encourages competing behavior delay the return of high levels of competing behavior in extinction? (2) Does partial reinforcement

IV

■?^«;„ a/>*

:4*

.;:■ ^' " , "!'”"4'-^^' ■' '' ' ^''■■

<t . . '■ •_,'^5y^

|9

, t

. r *• *

*' .. . 'Y - . •■ ' --wiv •^'*

•^1 ^• ji i»i 1 * i':m

•»'■*- I ;*M,:i' t)"st V.’"’

'iiT"

I •# *•'*▼» ^ -■■» » , * UJ ’ll 3l<tl«tkVl ^r^^,4iaf

•• .*• « » fiK . f. » . - . 1 ... . . ! *■•. fliiji lie |i^t«i ]f« IfHM^T t ■■ 4'«>>i

.'M ifra

'■ 'W ? •" . :y:xt>» '.

^ I {■'S i,'>fv^^^

5f^

■• >- , : ^;'!|pc,2

.■ I ■- 'V' •. : .1 ^ ■•

*-, '"‘^‘^^■.Vj.",' •- if ■*"

i I ‘*,,i ,. "iCci lisi^ ||»

<v‘

V. ?•■ -.4 V «

<’ 1-t^ , #4i| W (, ^4%: '■

'V"''

.H :- f 1, ^ i\r ^^^ >

V' *■

f ¥fi* y'^ *< }%.tnM.(i •«^M.'■*^■^■■)' T?si»r»5*hs.

in acquisition, which does retard extinction, do so through an increase in

competing behavior in acquisition?

Ten 23-hr, hungry rats were run in the alleyway to continuous reward (CRF), continuous reward with extra stimuli (CRF-S), or partial reward (PRF), for 59 trials. All were then run for at least 18 non- rewarded trials in extinction.

The primary results were: (1) extinction following continuously

rewarded acquisition did reflect a great return of competing and accessory behavior, of the same form as early in acquisition, consistent with the view of the alleyway as a selective-learning situation; (2) CRF-S rats showed slightly greater resistance to extinction than CRF rats; (3) the great resistance to extinction of the PRF rats was not a function of high levels of competing and accessory behavior in acquisition; (4) the PRF subjects showed much competing and accessory behavior and slow running in latter parts of the alleyway, and some continuously rewarded subjects showed similar behavior. This, plus observations in both experiments of recurrent behavior sequences across subjects and periods of atypically long run times for several subjects, suggests that an individual rat's behavior is not entirely independent of the behavior of other subjects run concurrently, though the mechanism underlying these interactions is not understood.

V

' ' ' '■? ' * I '';

If ;ju4.fff;U:tiil

x„.v; ■■ ,, ... ■■ ■.• . ' '• . ? . ' ■?<>" ■■ •■, . I

■,'.' . ,. '_ ..;. - *■ . -*•*

-4'

;iy rj: :-''^'-^vi .'

^

.. ; ■■' 1. . .. '■■■•v»,‘S9 S

' TiiTiiirtTiiiin' ^

■i’ 1 '’V ^ W '

.. ' ... : f -'s W,

,«r‘i -I

. :-,.f.v^.*' Viv-j,

. ' I . ..i'" r.i,'*TOI ,,

' t!;?** .i .:I '.>.?!r;... *-i. ••>*=• i»*j8r&’2»

-irak

vl.'.

.’orvv .!'

"m

' ' v»:>

-A.VV i

.U"’ '■„■ ’’If

■' ,' i'

j -^~.t

¥

A;,V

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

These experiments were supported by a Biomedical Sciences Sup- port Grant from Duke University to the author, and by grants from the National Institutes of Health and the National Science Foundation, J. E. R. Staddon, Principal Investigator.

I thank R. Courtney Newman for assistance with the pilot work pre- ceding the present experiments, and Sandra Ayres for help in coding some of the raw data of Experiment I. Dr. Carl J. Erickson and Dr. Norman Guttman kindly commented on proposals for these experiments, and Dr. Guttman gave valuable criticism of an earlier draft of Experiment I.

I thank my advisor Dr. Staddon for critical discussion and advice, and general intellectual stimulation, throughout this project.

My wife Meg helped with coding the raw data of Experiment II, pre- paring and checking rough figures and tables for both experiments, and critically reading parts of the manuscript. Less tangibly, she has helped through understanding, patience, and forebearance beyond all reason. There are no words to thank her.

J. E. K.

VI

' '■‘4'T

-4*> y.

r;;*:U«i' .>»!*; I;

'■ J .

•'k . :■ . " -V* - .-• ,'. ;., .j_ •■ •••.- .

"ill'*'-

T:-:***.

. ‘I' i > 1: A I<? 4 'a£» Wi

' , ' \ ' "\ ^ . . ' T'’ ‘‘ "

:.>: ti'J ' -x-.^

,r’ »•< 'il-t jj

:J-. ;ifc.f W../-a.li,' JU ^t4 i •**- *' i

'T -_i ♦.' )

:S;;

..Tx. <■„:,; .:*>

.•i -TlwiiJi'' 'f ii i>iijy|jtfii|y'i'^f'iii 'ill

A. -f^ , ■"

. iihd^e^llw *f ■%'

•■ "' ,,,-'.JU>T

.' r^'-. •*• ■' . s.-' tE“ .T.‘*.'lr>.>aM

<!. 4 . cr ' '•#;'

K’;;

. .'ii.

CONTENTS

ABSTRACT iii

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS vi

LIST OF TABLES viii

LIST OF FIGURES x

INTRODUCTION 1

EXPERIMENT I 10

Method 15

Results 28

Discussion 73

EXPERIMENT II 84

Method 87

Results 92

Discussion 127

REFERENCES 133

vii

LIST OF TABLES

Table Page

1. Major Categories of Alleyway Behavior 24

2. Summary of Behavior Changes in Acquisition

(Trials 1-25) for Both Groups 35

3. Range of DR Times in Acquisition and Throughout

Training 39

4. Effects of Amount of Reward on Acquisition 41

5. Proportion of Total CB and AB Trials on Which

Competing and Accessory Behavior Occurred

in Each Alle'^^ay Segment in Experiment I 60

6. Mean Run Time (Sec. ) per Alleyway Segment in

Experiment I 62

7. Range of Run Times (Sec. ) on DR Trials Showing

"Run" and Gallop Gaits 64

8. Proportion of CB Trials on Which Each CB Category

Occurred in Experiment I 66

9. Proportion of CB and AB Trials on Which Each AB

Category Occurred in Experiment I 69

10. Proportion of CB and AB Trials Showing Intra- and

Inter -Individual Behavior Sequences in Experiment I 71

11. Extra Stimuli for Gp CRF-S 90

12. Amount of Competing and Accessory Behavior During

Acquisition 99

viii

Table

Page

13. Proportion of Total CB and AB Trials in Acquisition

on Which Competing and Accessory Behavior Occurred in Each Alleyway Segment in Experi- ment II 101

14. Proportion of CB and AB Trials Showing Intra- and

Inter -Individual Behavior Sequences, and Propor- tion of Sequences Occurring in A^, During Acquisi- tion in Experiment II 103

15. Mean DR Time (Sec. ) per Alleyway Segment During

Acquisition in Experiment II 104

16. Proportion of CB Trials on Which Each CB Category

Occurred During Acquisition in Experiment II 106

17. Proportion of CB and AB Trials on Which Each AB

Category Occurred During Acquisition in Experi- ment II 109

18. Proportion of CB Trials on Which Each CB Category

Occurred During Extinction in Experiment II 114

19. Proportion of CB and AB Trials on Which Each AB

Category Occurred During Extinction in Experi- ment II 116

20. Mean Run Time (Sec. ) in Extinction for Gp CRF

and Gp CRF-S 120

21. Proportion of CB and AB Trials Showing Tntra- and

Inter -Individual Behavior Sequences, and Proportion of Sequences Occurring in A3, During Extinction in Experiment II 126

IX

f'iA '.' ll

-y v,r,v .-iftl'ft \ .'

r!*«'U *,|'

u*

■»4

(

.<

■'.'liW^ •V I, I i ,r

*teu;

, ...i i ; p:c»^ ^#lf ly

*!iP,-

-X ‘-V

" ' -‘- ,/t”,£^i f'. ;.■; /,

'' i' ■■':

1 ' >iv •*

. ff: l*f

.'.V " : .

... .-..m^,%.,.

^ius

i^-'-'f-.' '' ' V '

:*'.:-'-'r(t ': .nj

‘.■■IV'l.i>. ./

'WWsiirffilfiS

>'V

-V<.

.. '•‘•.*'^*'^11.,, V

,p-> Wv

,*t

^i.

M:

i'-Jii-S

>-!f

'w-

i#

LIST OF FIGURES

Figure

Page

1.

Reward Magnitude for Each Subject Throughout T raining.

22

2.

Behavior in Acquisition for Gp H.

30

3.

Behavior in Acquisition for Gp T.

32

4.

Behavior Following Upshifts in Amount of Reward for Gp H.

45

5.

Behavior Following Upshifts in Amount of Reward for Gp T.

47

6.

Behavior Following Downshifts in Amount of Reward for Gp H.

50

7.

Behavior Following Downshifts in Amount of Reward for Gp T.

52

8.

Behavior in the Presence of the Extra Stimulus for Gp H and Gp T.

58

9.

Behavior in Acquisition for Gp CRF-S.

94

10.

Behavior in Acquisition for Gp PRF.

97

11.

Behavior in Extinction for Gp CRF.

112

12.

Behavior in Extinction for Gp CRF-S.

119

13.

Behavior in Extinction for Gp PRF.

124

X

INTRODUCTION

In the familiar instrumental conditioning alleyway situation with rats, it is widely assumed, especially by S-R theorists following Hull (1943), that overall run time directly reflects the vigor of a single response, running. Presumably, when overall run time is longer, e.g., early in training, the rats are simply running more slowly (King, 1959; Logan, 1959; Marx &: Brownstein, 1963; Spence, 1956). However, little attention has been given to actually determining the form of the response measured by overall run time. S-R theorists sometimes acknowledge that behavior other than running may occur, especially early in training and during extinction, as incidental observations throughout the alleyway and maze -learning literature suggest (e.g., Bruce, 1935, 1937; Campbell & Meyer, 1971; Crespi, 1942; Graham & Gagne, 1940; Hammer, 1971; Hicks, 1911; Miller & Miles, 1936; Miller & Stevenson, 1936). When time-consuming "competing" behavior is occurring, the overall run time measure would obviously not directly reflect the rate of running. But, at least during continuously rewarded training, S-R theorists consider com- peting behavior an infrequent nuisance, extraneous to the basic process of learning demonstrated in the alleyway, viz. , the gradual strengthening

1

•" ^ *.i ;ittT^'3 'Vifl.J

'. ’■I

‘^,' ■■■’-’■'■■ <J^\v .•■. ^ ‘>, '■■■ ■■. -r-‘.. ..•, ', . -...^'sa-

■■ *'■*■ < -ii:’ ' ' I * !|i!fe^ iT^ w;

' 5 ^ i:

t '

, » I . .' '.'

' ^ / '3^

' ■' i:.' .. ' T* :,r '*i|fi.t2,;r)^ 4(^1'' %i^v' I ft*? ^ ,■*;*

^ ,., . '4%

'J^ . .

■,S

* -

;r '-V^^ . ••■•,■;••■•' ' V ^v' >V?!' V-*aF.§

) V

■■■"■■• .'■ '■ ::' " »■ ' •■ ' >'' ;.4 >' ' "^ '■=

, * '.I'M . v'V '4 *'■■

' ' "“'- -W: ::

I ' '•>

r’ ^

.-.-lid'- :X- A'i

*■ '\. ''V j , , . '

X.'J'jr, * ^

* S' . > S'l

a' 4'

,^Jt. ;v^-

1 < ■% ■i- 4 .,-» *>♦ , ' V-’ ^ *^^^2

■“ ' ; ^4 4K'

2

(increase in speed) of a single response (running) with practice (Spence, 1956, pp. 37-39, 116-117; 1958, p, 74; I960, pp, 122-123). Thefree- operant Skinner box situation is also widely considered to allow study of the strengthening of a single response in relative isolation (cf ., Skinner, 1938, p. 55).

On the other hand, some theorists have argued that every learning situation is a multiple response situation, and that instrumental responses such as running are "all -or -none" responses without an intensity, or amplitude, dimension (Bindra, 1961; Estes, 1950, 1959; Guthrie, 1959; Knarr & Collier, 1962; Premack, 1965). In this view a long run time would reflect, not slow running, but a low relative frequency of "bursts" of running at a constant intra-burst rate, and a corresponding high rela- tive frequency of competing behavior. Acquisition in the alleyway would consist of an increase in the relative frequency of running at the expense of competing behavior rather than an increase in the rate of running; extinction would reflect an increase in the frequency of competing behav- ior and a decrease in the frequency of running, rather than a decrease in the rate of running. According to this general "response -competition" account (Bindra, 1961; Estes, 1950, 1959; Guthrie, 1935, 1959), compet- ing behavior is not random "noise, " to be eliminated or at least ignored (cf . , Spence, 1956, I960), but is an orderly and integral part of instru- mental learning and performance.

A handful of alleyway experiments directly concerned with the role

> ^

j j

. ‘I >\ t .. :'f <•' -

)■ W '*

.. A-J) •■;;

h >*. V

, , I »»•'»>■

Tin

. -■' s

^T"'V' '■ - V'©:'.*,.

»• r '^i- -' * -3f |. : «af*Si

' '■ ■’■ . -

'•■'■‘I

l.J; ••,

.-4,;' i?5:, *:-;«!

V »

' '• .r .- -Xr r ->1,^ : .m

,.rv •'. » .'TV,.

bfe ' - t-A ^

•-•. ‘.‘ , <l>!i ';•••••' . ...vi/.V:- » T m-'Ot'SiS

'• : " .... -'>'. .«iC.'.: ^ m

i% >■■ ^

m

I. » . *

-■■f.’i»*[» '-T

3

of competing behavior in continuously rewarded instrumental learning and performance offer some support for the response- competition view. ^

They have shown that competing behavior is not infrequent and limited to early stages of acquisition, but may occur throughout training. Further, they have established that the decrease in overall run time in acquisition, and with larger reward or more severe deprivation, is accompanied by a corresponding decrease in the amount of competing behavior (Bindra, 1963; Cicala, 1961; Cotton, 1953; Kello, unpublished experiment; King, 1959; Marx & Brownstein, 1963; McCoy & Marx, 1965; Pereboom & Crawford,

^Although the main concern here is with alleyway experiments, there are also data in the free-operant literature which strongly support the response-competition view. Thus Gilbert (1958) showed that the differ- ences in overall rate of bar-pressing in highly trained rats as a function of percent concentration of sucrose were due almost entirely to differences in the amount of time spent responding. When a rat was responding it bar- pressed at a nearly constant rate regardless of the magnitude of reward; at higher magnitudes it simply responded for a greater proportion of the total time (more frequent response bursts, longer bursts, shorter pause between bursts). Similarly, Blough (1963) found that under a wide range of conditions, including changes in the length of the average interreinforce- ment interval, stimulus generalization tests, and extinction, pigeons responded at a basic within-burst rate of 2-3 keypecks per sec. When overall rate changed, this basic rate remained the same, but the latency to bursts and the frequency and duration of bursts changed appropriately. (See also arguments by Baum & Rachlin, 1969; Catania, 1961; Premack, 1965; Premack & Schaeffer, 1962; Rachlin, 1972. )

Although it is sometimes explicitly assumed in free-operant work that when bar-pressing or key-pecking are not occurring some other (competing) behavior is (cf. , DRO schedules, Reynolds, 1961), operant studies dealing with the structure of overall response rate have not focused on competing behavior. Overall response rate in these studies is analyzed in terms of bursts of operant responses and "pauses." The highly auto- mated free-operant methodology has encouraged observation of behavior even less than the discrete-trials alleyway approach (cf. , Skinner, 1938, pp. 58-59).

. sTT

-.. ,-ii >

. ’i '^ *

7r .VJi

I » ■.'♦* J

•.-

f^W.

w...i<*^-‘^k.

\t '.1

,-j

, ,| ti".'-\... '.' i\>' w W, J J^^-'-r." .

' '^ , . ‘J

lif;- ;;

1^

« tit,

'^m

-it'’ 1'

•■’. '''.*«^'-* 'i* -rran

- 'i : , ' ' i.1 ‘i iiSr »|;’:r.;f - ,

JJ-.

'5' 'f ^

-vjf

'.yfi

'™. ’■.%t’'' ■■■'.;- ■'

■, ' "■ W = ,

'M

i'v/M#.

4

1958; Smoot, 1964). However, each of these studies which has examined a "pure" measure of running with competing behavior eliminated (all but Bindra, 1963, and McCoy & Marx, 1965) has found significant, though smaller, changes in this measure as well. The studies differ widely in the extent to which variation in the amount of competing behavior alone can account for the variation in the overall run time measure.

Several weaknesses in these studies suggest that the obtained changes in the vigor of running without competing behavior may be arti- factual, and therefore that a simple response-competition view may best account for alleyway learning and performance. Two of the studies suf- fered quite obvious measurement defects (cf. , Marx h Brownstein, 1963). Cotton (1953) found that, while the decrease in overall run time with an increase in hunger drive was largely due to a decrease in the amount of competing behavior, the vigor of running without competing behavior also increased within a narrow range. However, Cotton reports that under the milder deprivation conditions a high proportion of the fastest trials, pre- sumably with no competing behavior, was lost due to subjects' jumping over photocells. This was particularly the case for 0-hr. deprivation, with up to 22. 5% of the fastest trials being lost for some subjects. And although it is not reported which levels of deprivation differed signifi- cantly from each other in terms of vigor or running, inspection of Cotton's Fig. 1 (1953, p. 192) suggests that the overall significant differences were probably due entirely to the longer times at 0-hr. deprivation.

5^!'

I'p

Ilf ^ I * (, >

■' 'v'*.*,

■>1'

'• .; 1 u;. *ri f iWH>>;»--*«»3'.4^*

s.v

>i> ■•*• ■*■

:iW'

>-

. ,. ;.4*;''.vi( I ; ■'«'

#3^1®

.w^

*©«”*

•> ) t * '

i'j^ 7- >■ ; *•■*.%

■X.

If ' !;• ."*» Tv

.',-‘'V'!l„

..i-. '^ ■■■ -M- >..v'’ii

■':. ??| I IV .'t'''' f r' V

:,fa:rv

m

,w'

, ,1 »if" ' I '1.

-

'4'""

iTik-.

- v,„

•" '_ •■., v:'- ■• ■■■ I'j^v .■■ '‘j,’ :;

'Tip

■7

' J

r-

,0''

5

Pereboom and Crawford (1958) found small but significant changes in the pure measure of the instrumental response both in acquisition and as a function of reward magnitude. In their study, however, when a subject retraced, the time taken for him to return to where he had initially stopped and turned around was included in the measure of the instrumental response for that trial. Given that retracing is more frequent early in training and with small reward, "pure" run time would obviously be longer under these conditions whether subjects actually ran more slowly or not, as Pereboom and Crawford acknowledge.

A central problem with each of the available studies of alleyway competing behavior is that little attention has been paid to specifying clearly what constitutes competing behavior, and then to applying the criteria uniformly (cf. , McCoy & Marx, 1965, p. 356). The criteria that have been used vary extensively from study to study, and probably do not in any case encompass all behavior incompatible with forward progress in the alleyway. Even the fundamental criterion of competing behavior, viz., that the subject stop, is not always explicit (e.g., Pereboom & Crawford, 1958). At any rate, since defining properties of criterial behaviors are almost never given, it is not always clear what constitutes a stop and what proportion of the stops in one study would be rated as stops in another study. Also, since trials were never filmed or video - recorded in these studies, there was no way to review difficult decisions, and no way to ensure that application of the criteria for competing

«wii»t ■Ja>«

•^1

kk

" ■■■■„, i

r-^i>.‘'' r'i." ■<ii

' 'fc»“

t.

I ^ - 't«J I ' ■" ,.

;♦

ffii.'i.

. i>.

yjj .'

' tm ....

'♦e’t''®'' j.^'.,5e

' ,- '•a4

" ■r4tfW'

'., . y-. ■■ ^'

yl^^i

bet

•j- rJ

'■“‘I" - . . '*^A- (^■'•'■/‘ ' ' '■'

■,, , ■„>

im

m

‘its’'

It

. 401''

>■• ■> i /'.,

, ' S5,

'''W:

•<»*a

•f®.

iv^iTf r

■SL

k '^1

I C®S^’ ,•*'•' :'',.A.i.

Ji

W

..tO

Xi V '"hUi-

"-VTlBrn

4iw^t /,y

6

behavior, whatever they were, did not change in the course of the experi- ment as the observer became more familiar with the behavior of rats in alleyways. It is likely then that some indeterminate amount of the obtained variation in the vigor of running in each of these studies can be attributed to including time-consuming competing behavior in the "pure" measure of running.

It has also been suggested that there is an intermediate class of "competing" behavior which is not overtly incompatible with running, but which may occur concurrently with running and slow it down, e. g. , sniff- ing the floor while running forward (Kello, unpublished experiment; Marx & Brownstein, 1963; Pereboom & Crawford, 1958; Smoot, 1964). This be- havior has not been separately studied, due to difficulties in specifying it (Marx & Brownstein, 1963, p. 188). With the possible exception of the King (1959) experiment, it has simply been included in the "pure" measure of running. If this behavior occurs where full competing behavior is likely to occur, then including it in the "pure" measure of running would further spuriously amplify the changes seen in this measure.

A final specific weakness of these studies of competing behavior in the alleyway is that two different pure measures of the instrumental run- ning response have been used: a "forward progress" measure, which equals the total time on a trial minus the time spent in competing behavior (King, 1959; Marx & Brownstein, 1963; Pereboom & Crawford, 1958; Smoot, 1964), and a "direct run" measure, which is simply the total time

i

bit uS -

|, Jr- -III** *1.4 Ma) ^ if'' "'

!,’ *r.: 1

■•'i' *t)i H">/''-

f 1 fj'jj' 1

).i .I'.X:^, '■ * ■■■ I ' ,

.■ 'to . .

< * 4 I- ^ fj‘*

' *«.» ?■!;!(■<■ SfiJf’ ..;

t. : •,|.-

' * “4

,/L, iUotii

, i ■' . WJ

<V . o •«<.■« ifr. V-. « ■»! - *■'>., ^

''( ' .**

■;v. C,„

r4«'4^

' H-',;

. . .... ... ...;J'

' - f ’, -J, ;,„i

; > -<•' ■■?','•» vl '^*'i i(-i4*. ,J

7

on those trials on which no competing behavior occurs (Cicala, 1961; Cotton, 1953; Marx & Brownstein, 1963). The two measures are not strictly comparable. It has been shown that the direct run measure varies much less than the forward progress measure, and it has been argued on several grounds that direct run time is a clearer measure of running without competing behavior than is forward progress time (Kello, unpub- lished experiment; Marx & Brownstein, 1963).

The first experiment sought to correct the deficiencies of previous studies of alleyway competing behavior, with rigorous and uniformly applied criteria of competing behavior and the instrumental response.

The purpose was to determine whether changes in competing behavior alone can account for changes in overall run time, as the response- competition view claims. Also, the range of variables to which this gen- eral type of analysis has been applied was extended in this experiment.

There are two more general problems with the previous studies of competing behavior to which the first experiment was also addressed. First, it is not clear to what extent the results describe the behavior of any single subject. Alleyway studies have traditionally dealt with group- averaged data, and the studies of competing behavior discussed here are no exception. It is generally conceded that it is impossible to argue from group-averaged curves to single-subject behavior, though quite simple to argue in the reverse direction (Bakan, 1954; Estes, 1956; Hayes, 1953; Sidman, 1952; Spence, 1956). Yet, it is also quite clear that learning

8

theorists intend their statements to apply to the behavior of single sub- jects (Estes, 1956; Hayes, 1953, 269; Skinner, 1961; Spence, 1956, pp. 60-61). Thus 'while interested in the behavior of the individual, learn- ing theorists' accounts of behavior in the alleyway are based almost entirely on group -averaged data, which may not in fact accurately repre- sent the behavior of any individual subject (cf. , Rashotte, Adelman, &; Dove, 1972). It should be clear that the role of competing behavior in instrumental learning and performance can be understood only through an analysis of the behavior of individual subjects.

A second problem is the lack of direct obser-vational study of the whole range of behavior in the alleyway, and in other learning situations (Bindra, 1961; Guthrie, 1959; Mackintosh, 1955; Staddon & Simmelhag, 1971; but see Zener, 1937). The only interest in alle-yway competing behavior has been to identify it in order to remove it, to determine whether a pure measure of the instrumental response varies with the over- all rate measure. In the absence of a careful, direct observational analy- sis of the structure of alleyway behavior, basic questions remain as to what competing behavior is, and how orderly it is, common assumptions being that it is simply "random" or "spontaneous" activity (see Bindra, 1961; Hicks, 1911; Spence, 1956), or behavior directly elicited by novel stimuli (Bindra, 1961; Hinde, 1970; Mackintosh, 1955; Pereboom, 1957; Spence, 1956).

Most broadly stated, the aim of the first experiment was to provide

.1 ««.

V - , '"' : ; ' : ', .:/

< •■■4v*4 'f« V ' ^-*»*

, .i._\ ^ '. ■■"*„,■ .MW

r,ik

'■‘ifiSft’-- " ' --'^-

-.-> '^ '4-' **■■', t ‘--i '.. 1!!' ,*''!^

, .. .,

,(cr'' - ' '•M . tl~»

ft ■,( in •< V / itt

.... y-n

, ^ ■* " ;:‘ A‘. : "v ‘■^V .;■ ^

( :

.'■jt*.

'■ -‘iv Ttl.

■•"•■ife

9

a descriptive account of the trial -by -trial behavior of individual rats run- ning to reward in the alleyway under a variety of experimental conditions, to better understand the form and nature of competing behavior in the alleyway, as well as its role in determining measured overall run time.

EXPERIMENT I

The first experiment studied the behavior of individual rats running to reward in an alleyway as a function of number of rewarded trials (acquisition), amount of reward during acquisition, shifts in amount of reward, proximity to reward, and deprivation state and associated reward. The last three variables require further comment.

Reward magnitude shifts. When amount of food reward is increased or decreased in the alleyway situation, overall run time has been found to change abruptly in the appropriate direction (Crespi, 1942; Zeaman, 1949). It has been argued that, along with other variables which affect the over- all run time measure, the effects of shifts in reward magnitude may be mediated through competing behavior and not by changes in the rate of running (Hammer, 1971; Pereboom, 1957; Pereboom & Crawford, 1958). There is some informal evidence that downshifts in amount of reward may indeed engender competing behavior in the alleyway and that upshifts may reduce or eliminate it (Crespi, 1942). But the role of competing behavior in reward magnitude shift effects has not been studied directly, and it is therefore not clear to what extent competing behavior alone can account for these changes in overall run time.

10

11

The classical studies showing large and abrupt changes in run time with changes in reward magnitude generally shifted after 15-25 trials at 1 trial per day. A recent study of magnitude shift effects which shifted after 50 trials at 1 trial per day found that at this late stage of training a 25 -fold increase in amount had ordy a very slight, though consistent, effect on early segment start and run time; a comparable decrease in amount engendered only a slight and transient increase in run time (Hammer,

1971). Hammer informally observed that the subjects receiving small reward exhibited much competing behavior early in training. Although no quantitative data were presented, she suggested that the long and variable run times of the small reward subjects were due to this competing behav- ior. She also observed that later in training, when this behavior had dropped out for the small reward subjects, their run times were consistent and very close to the asymptotic level attained (much earlier in training) by the large reward subjects. Hammer argued that, since run time given no competing behavior does not differ greatly for large or small reward subjects (cf. , also Pereboom & Crawford, 1958), large and abrupt changes in run time with reward magnitude shifts early in training must reflect changes in the amount of competing behavior. Later in training, when competing behavior has dropped out and only the relatively invariant instrumental response is occurring, magnitude shifts will have little effect.

In light of Hammer's hypothesis, the present experiment ran rats to "large, " "medium, " and "small" amounts of reward and shifted at various

S'., ..S'

13. JS,;

J ■'

.1 . -f ^

I is*

IF' ■*■'

r >- ‘1

1 n*

- ■■ ,:#:■# '' '-.y if

-%:4

i't '■',, 'vv ‘•as** ?i:

4-Sf

. ■.* : ,’( ,,. •••I*. ..-.s *. . . q^|||pf<j|p>’|;^'.ai.,

'I* f .1. Siv*' t ^^0.#V.■M^ llj tti

[:nL«i

■:.-W4<'

■kfa

.■;■■■

|j.;^vv,:.:4rt?-''

i'lls'u-'i W

- ' ’.'. '"’’ >''••■ ■*'. _, tii

._ 1

■■ -S

. '. V I,' :■: "■•» ■/. ;^-< '

j ..(_'iit!.‘'Ai. I

t^l’'*'i6!l-J

..It ’.!' ’iv' l’ ’“" ■' '* '■■ ... liuffi. V '''

ri»‘. :-*

ii, i

i-r ' irtfe "., ■'*■

V.

12

stages of training, looking at both running and competing behavior through- out. The purpose was to determine whether large-scale changes in over- all run time with reward magnitude shifts reflect changes in competing behavior and not in the vigor of running, and whether such changes occur early but not late in training.

Proximity to reward. Although the locus of alleyway competing behavior has not been studied directly, it has been suggested that in some situations competing behavior tends to occur mainly in early parts of the alleyway, farthest from reward (Bindra, 1963; Marx & Brownstein, 1963; Miller & Miles, 1935), and incidental observations throughout the litera- ture support this suggestion (Bruce, 1937; Campbell & Meyer, 1971;

Crespi, 1942; Hammer, 1971; Hicks, 1911; Kello, unpublished observation). One obvious implication of these observations is that the "goal gradient" (Hull, 1934), viz. , faster run time closer to the goal, may be an "artifact" of competing behavior (King, 1959; Marx & Brownstein, 1963). Data pre- sented by King (1959) offer some support for this possibility, although this experiment suffered the weaknesses outlined previously (pp. 4-8). King showed that the slope of the goal gradient was reduced when competing behavior was eliminated, but there were no data on the locus of competing behavior. Further evidence that the goal gradient may be due to an early segment bias of competing behavior comes from observations that the gradient is sharpest early in training and with smaller reward (Bruce,

1937; Crespi, 1942; Hull, 1934), and that a sharp goal gradient returns

13

early in extinction (Hull, 1934; Miller & Miles, 1935). All these situa- tions presumably involve a high level of competing behavior.

The present experiment examined the locus of competing behavior and the role of competing behavior in determining the goal gradient.

Deprivation state and associated reward. Most laboratory studies of learning and performance in animals, including those discussed here, have used hungry animals responding for food. The results of such studies have been generalized freely to other " need- reiniorcer” relationships under two related assumptions: (a) that all "drives" have equivalent effects

on behavior (Brown, 1961; Campbell & Cicala, 1962; Petrinovich & Bolles, 1954; Tapp, 1969), which is implicit in Hull's (1943) "big D," generalized drive; (b) that all reinforcers operate in the same way in all learning situa- tions (Seligman, 1970; Shettleworth, 1972; Teitelbaum, 1966). These assumptions have been borne out in many situations for thirsty animals performing for water (e. g. , Jenkins & Arnold, 1968; Kintsch, 1962;

McCoy Marx, 1965; Skinner, 1938; Weinstock, 1958; Zimmerman, 1971), in that results comparable to those with hungry, food-rewarded subjects are found. But in some cases, which have received little attention (cf. , Shettleworth, 1972), the results are not the same. For example, it appears that simultaneously food- and water -deprived rats running to water in an alleyway may show no "frustration effect" (Levy & Seward, 1969), and thirsty rats may show only a very slight decremental effect of long delays of water reward in the alleyway (Logan & Spanier, 1970).

j I Vml

.m-

v ifl

Mii^ ~ ’:s '•il

..*jr,:- J1 , ^f- ' , . . ■{'' ^:‘-v

':. i

X '-' i- „,, " ■'

» . r —'a.

&

.. .. 4 y

- .;. €=^vi-

. i A :; •ajjfl-

t (

i-^'!

Jil

M"'). ' 'arjS

^ ^ \>V _ ' .

'■ '■‘■■'f'f Qtr;jilfc •' ;■ t

V*

V ;,v,

’^'■' '-'4 '^^■ -

.-^S -i-j 'T » ■^. | .tf*; lit*-.

- . i-

■1.

t*>ft If,? ?4» 4

;■■ . «• ■■;

•-'i*'-'^^- i... ■■■ ■* ■. '-J? ijc'

^ ,t- '

'.i.vjf».s:,v5

'•*'* d ■^‘V-* . •'.!>'# •♦ , '■' !• •, •!-. •'Vli (*#'>»» i

.Ti

jS’.-.;

. %\

Ti:

1*^;;

14

Also, in the free -operant bar -pressing situation there may be no "partial reinforcement effect" with water (Macdonald & de Toledo, 1972),

There are several other lines of evidence suggesting behavioral dif- ferences between hungry, food -rewarded and thirsty, water -rewarded animals. Thus it has been shown that it is easier to train hungry than thirsty rats to alternate for reward in a T -maze, while thirsty rats form a position habit more readily than hungry ones (Bolles, 1958a; Petrinovich & Bolles, 1954). In a number of "general activity" situations (e.g,, run- ning wheel, stabilimeter cage, bar -pressing for light), thirsty rats show much less activity than hungry rats deprived for the same number of hours or at the same percentage of ad lib body weight (Campbell &; Cicala, 1962; Hall, 1955; Tapp, 1969). Also, in an alleyway study using thirsty rats, Kintsch (1962) incidentally reported observing very little competing behav- ior, even with minimal deprivation and small reward. These results sug- gest overall that thirsty rats may show less variability in their behavior than hungry rats. On the other hand, Bruce (1935) found that 24 -hr. thirsty rats clearly showed more variability and performed more poorly throughout training in a multiple unit maze than 24 -hr. hungry rats. And at least two alleyway experiments dealing with competing behavior have reported, contrary to Kintsch (1962). that competing behavior does occur throughout training for thirsty rats (Bindra, 1963; McCoy & Marx, 1965).

The present experiment examined the behavior of thirsty rats run- ning to water under the same conditions as the hungry rats running to

K

15

food, comparing the running and competing behavior of the two groups at each stage of training. It has not previously been determined to what extent competing behavior can account for variations in overall run time for thirsty rats running to water with any of the variables studied here. Further, it was of interest to see if the categories of behavior shown by thirsty subjects would differ from those shown by hungry subjects (cf . , Petrinovich & Bolles, 1954; Shettleworth, 1972; Tapp, 1969).

Method

Subjects

The subjects were 12 male Long-Evans hooded rats, approximately 100 days old on experimenta.1 day 1.

Apparatus

The subjects were run in a straight alleyway, 1.90 m. long from the start door to the last photocell, with internal dimensions of 12.7 cm. by 12.7 cm. The apparatus was constructed of plywood and was covered throughout with a hinged top of clear Plexiglas. Attached to the alleyway proper were a 22.9 cm. long startbox (SB) and a 20.3 cm. long goalbox (GB), both L -shaped. The floor of the entire apparatus was painted a dull white. The walls of SB were painted flat black, while the walls of GB and the alleyway were left unpainted. Four pairs of photocells and light sources were spaced at 61 -cm. intervals in the alleyway, dividing it into three consecutive 61 -cm. (2 ft.) segments, A^^, A^, and A^, from SB to

16

GB. The first photocell was placed 7.0 cm. from the clear Plexiglas guillotine -type start door which separated SB from the alley. A similar Plexiglas door was located in A^, 8.9 cm. before the last photocell. This door, normally up, was dropped to prevent retracing after a rat had entered GB. Both doors were operated manually, by strings. The photo- cells were wired to Standard electric timers to give run times accurate to .0,1 sec. for each of the three segments of the alley.

The alleyway was illuminated by an overhead incandescent light fix- ture, shielded by white Plexiglas, and running the length of the apparatus. The output of this fixture was reduced so that illuminance throughout the alleyway at floor level was quite dim (2. 1-2. 3 ft. candles), but sufficient for video -recording. Masking noise (speech noise) was on continuously during sessions, at 70 db.

A Concord TV camera was mounted from the ceiling in line with the alleyway, 61 cm. behind the end of GB and approximately 1.90 m. up from the floor. The swivel mounting allowed the camera to sweep down the length of the alleyway to continuously follow subjects' movements throughout. A mirror was mounted above and behind SB so that the sub- jects' movements in SB, partly obscured from direct camera view, could be continuously monitored. A white curtain was suspended approximately 61 cm. behind GB such that the experimenter’s movements in operating the camera and the start and retrace doors were concealed from subjects in the alleyway. The camera was connected with a Concord video-tape

■■

■f

'f'i!

..y9

. /'V"]

^ -k. . J

» 4 * . ..-4.* 1 j- i 4 . i

* •! ■'»

' «31J

4

4' *

»

,1 I , 1 4

V .jI'. '

.if Ml',

■:.-,X

'n'l'

■"' •vi'-'

r: itii

'■:.■■■ ri'-..- = '.'i: t^}'

AW»*“^ .'

. .■'w ' ' «« f''

s'l" ^!!!^' -.4 i' ,: '!'N’' 'ft'»‘. V

17

recorder and monitor.

The alleyway and all associated recording equipment were located in a semi-soundproof experimental room some distance away from the animal colony.

Pretraining

After arrival in the laboratory the subjects were kept in individual cages in 24-hr. light throughout the experiment. For 6 days they were given ad lib food (Purina chow) and water, and were handled for 2-3 min. and weighed daily. The subjects were then randomly divided into two groups, "hungry" (Gp H) and "thirsty" (Gp T). From the 7th day of pre- training to the end of the experiment, Gp H was fed only for 1 hr. per day, and Gp T was watered for only 1 hr. per day. Gp H had continuous access to water and Gp T had continuous access to food. On day 7 both groups were separately transported in their home cages, without food or water, to the experimental room, at the same time of day as they would later be run in the experiment proper (Gp H at 9:00 a.m. and Gp T at 11:00 a.m.). They were exposed to the experimental room, which was set up for an experimental session, and to typical within -session equipment noises such as the activation of the photocell -clock circuitry, operation of the guillo- tine doors, and operation of the TV camera and video-tape recorder, inter- mittently, for 1-2 hr. Pretraining days 8-l6 differed from day 7 only in that subjects were also handled and gentled in the experimental room on

18

these days. The rats received their 1-hr. daily ration of the appropriate deprived substance approximately 10-15 min. after they were returned from the experimental room to their colony. Body weights were moni- tored daily throughout the experiment.

Experimental procedure

Prior to the start of experimental training, subjects within the two groups were randomly assigned to either of three reward magnitudes -- large, medium, or small. For Gp H, small reward was 1 .045 g. Noyes pellet, medium was 4 pellets (. 18 g. ), and large was 16 pellets (. 72 g. ). For Gp T, small reward was 2 drops (. 17 ml.) of cool water from a stan- dard eyedropper, medium was 6 drops (.50 ml.), and large was 18 drops (1. 5 ml.). There were two rats in each group at each of the three reward magnitudes. The particular magnitudes used for large, medium, and small were selected on the basis of work with pilot subjects, which showed that the consummatory times for food and water at each of the three levels were approximately equivalent. Rewards were presented in food cups, which were placed in the short arm of GB and were not visible from the alley.

On experimental day 1 each subject was run one trial to the appro- priate magnitude of the appropriate reward. One trial was also run on day 2, and two trials on each of days 3 and 4. Thereafter, three trials per day were run, to the conclusion of the experiment at day 27. Within

19

each of the groups the daily running order of the six subjects was random- ized. On each experimental day a group was brought to the experimental

. i

room, in which all equipment which would be operating during the session was on, and was left for a minimum of 10 min. while the floor of SB, GB, and the alley were sponged with a weak solution of Lysol cleaner in warm water, then dried with paper towels. At the beginning of a trial the appro- priate subject was placed into SB by hand. The experimenter quickly turned on the video -recording equipment and positioned himself behind the curtain. From here he operated the camera and the start and retrace doors. When the rat had been in SB a minimum of 10 sec., and had been near the start door and oriented toward it for 3 sec., the start door was raised. If the subject began retracing back to SB after entering the alley- way, the start door was quietly lowered to prevent his reentry into SB. When the rat activated the last photocell and entered GB, the retrace door was dropped behind him and the video -recording equipment was turned off. After consuming the reward, the subject was immediately removed from GB and returned to his cage. Times were recorded, clocks reset, GB baited, and the floor of the apparatus was again sponged with Lysol and dried. The intertrial interval for a given subject was always 15-21 min. Prior to each session the water was removed from the cages of Gp H, and the food was removed from the cages of Gp T. After a session the sub- jects were returned to the colony, where Gp H was again given ad lib water, and Gp T ad lib food. After approximately 5 min. the subjects

20

receiving small and medium amounts of reward were fed or watered the difference between the amount they had just received in the alleyway and the amount the large-reward subjects had received, in order to control for differential weight gain. Ten min. after this had been consumed, all sub- jects were allowed 1-hr, access to the appropriate deprived substance.

At trial 25 one of the small-reward subjects in Gp H and one in Gp T, randomly selected prior to trial 1, were shifted to the large reward magnitude (see Fig. 1 for the reward magnitude of each subject throughout training). Similarly, one of the two large reward-subjects in each group was shifted to the small amount at this stage of training. All subjects were then run to trial 54 with no further change in reward conditions. However, immediately after the session ending with trial 33, insecticide was sprayed in parts of the experimental room. This change in stimulus conditions affected the subsequent behavior of some of the rats in the alleyway such that it was not possible to obtain relatively stable individual run times, desired before the next shift in reward magnitudes, until around trial 45-54. At trial 55 all of the rats were shifted to a small or large amount. The previously unshifted large-reward subject in each group, the subject shifted to large reward at trial 25, and one of the two medium- reward subjects chosen at random were all shifted to the small amount; the previously unshifted-small reward subject in each group, the subject shifted to small reward at trial 25, and the remaining medium-reward subject were all shifted to the large amount. Again at trial 67, all of the

mi

Jimnk-.

*mr

t^«49«iiuf ii''

t'V5 ' ■■ ?■ •■ ;

frj7.4 f ^aC^’isai

.fk i:‘

I I •'

V- f'

T. f

.b'

r I

rf ■“l’’•

r * i,! !■:, f ■i».i:'ii^,';ji . '• / » t . I. 4-1 3-^f#'

/ ' f.'.

•i :

4 »

tt i (J.O .:

r?ti3

i' ; iw. .■„. =..;

^ > . . •. -i tir‘: '!** '4. ' , it

4 PC

*' 4MI'

; : 1 1 1>) HAK - ' »^i'^

nr>r

ti t e^ ;ai'

'■» '.‘v-f *v-'':;t'«

'* '" . TT W " 'lip X"’^

< 41 ?> *

%...

•le'-

t-i

M> V*»J

1,

t/t-' i

■■»• ,u>.

■iim

■l)'/(.t / '.-i 'l

"■T^:" .',(v;s.; '"’^i

.-»•■ tu :’•.■'<:■> if' <ii^i

■>%.l

>;8

■«*t' .*ii

,*t*j v tIILj

>

»^.r'^

.i- •**,:*■

f/

I 'fc •7Stw^n:..i

i:

i'

{

I

)'

(

[

I

' >

,. i

21

Fig. 1. Reward Magnitude for Each Subject Throughout Training.

22

Subjects

Trials

1 . . .

Gp H 101 (sm)

. 109 (sm)

25 . . .

(Ig)

55 . . . 67 ... 75

(sm) ^Clg)

(Ig) ►(sm)

105 (med) 113 (med)

(Ig)-

(sm)-

(sm)

^-(Ig)

108 (Ig)

no (Ig)

-► (sm)

(sm)

(Ig)-

(Ig)'

(sm)

Gp T 102 (sm) (Ig) ►(sm)

112 (sm) (Ig) ►(sm) (Ig)

103 (med)— (sm) (Ig)

107 (med) (Ig) (sm)

106 (Ig) (sm) (Ig) ►(sm)

111 (Ig) (sm) (Ig) >

23

subjects currently receiving small reward were shifted to large, and vice versa. All were run to the conclusion of the experiment at trial 75 under these reward conditions. Thus some small - and some large -reward subjects were shifted early, and all of the subjects were shifted at middle and late stages of training.

Data analysis

In addition to the automatic recording of the overall run time sepa- rately for each of the three alleyway segments, each trial was video- taped. Table 1 presents a system for categorizing the alleyway behavior of rats, which accounted for virtually all of the subjects' within -trial time in pilot observations (Kello, unpublished experiment) and in the present experiments. Based on this system, each trial was classified as either of three mutually exclusive and exhaustive types. A trial on which a stop, hesitate, or freeze occurred between the first and last photocells, and thus the subject's hind legs ceased forward progress, was scored as a full com- peting behavior (CB) trial; a trial on which the subject elongated from the start -box and activated the first photocell before moving his hind legs was also scored as a CB trial. A trial on which no stop, hesitate, freeze, or initial elongation from SB into A^^ occurred, but on which the subject showed some other classifiable behavior concurrently with running to GB, e.g., AH, S(F), S{W), was scored as an "accessory" behavior (AB) trial.

A trial on which the subject showed only running, and no classified

Major Categories of Alleyway Behavior

24

0

fH

01

rH

fH

u

to

o

Q

ttO

c

|H > O

CO

tuO

O

r*H

"xi

oJ

bfl

0

O

u

rd

bO

52

r4

o

a

o

o

o

I

■xl

d

CD

+->

VH

o

d

<u

a

(U

>

o

a

n5

fH

o

>-M

o

CO

to ■x)

0) fH

ccJ

. ^ ^

o fH

til

(Ci

d ^ M

,0 42 fU

r-l *rH

+H 1 S.

d >' o

d a o a

O I

J-J J-J d (Tj to

+H

CO CO

CO bO (U CO

bO

CD

fH

c\3

O

fH

4->

O

d

CO

(U

r-H O

X5

'HH 5/1

4-> r— I CO T}

o d

CO

bJD

d

iH

d d d

fH

m O

d o

« iH

4J

'4-> d

d CO

O CO

u o

U

o ■:g

CO o IT) fQ

CD

(U

a

o

CO

CO

bJO

O

I— I

(U

fH

o

H->

d

(U

CO

d

o

d

d

fH

o

'HH

CO

bo •':i

m

CO O

o

fH

bJO

2 ^

5-1 0)

dn > X!

fH

ccj

fH

o

(cJ O

CO

"HH

bJO O

b£i

d

u

<v

CO

IT)

fH

d

o" a

I HH

H-J

CTj

b£)

d

o

CD

d) d rd o Ph >HH d

° d

i^t: ^

b d CO

do,

d

in

pH .t; xJ d CO

o fi o

rQ 42 rd

C

H-H

CU

d h,

rH fl }h

2 22

-C-* <H-H O

^ CJ

^ rd 4->

fH <hH d

cc5 O p,

4->

d X) - <U fi d d M

P > d

?, u ^

S 2 -2

^ g>2

.5

o5 ^ CO

^ 2 -

CO fH

bJO -f* fH

CD d

Ti CD

fi

fH P ^

O P rd

h a

o ^

p, o ^ o ^

<D

O

fH Xi O

(C5 fi jf

P d O

^ a

fi ^ O

CD

4l

r , , '+-1

O ^

VH CO fH

O

d

MH

r^

2 p

•H CJ

a ^

(U HJ

^ P

0) 5h

'zl

H-> d

HH (U O rd H H->

d

o

•rH

o P d

2 2 rd

p a

^ 2

d ^

•H

bX)

d

r4 fH fH

d

u

x! P

O fi 42

►r-5 r-'

u

P bjO Q

bO P o d X3 ^ d _ d

■gS 5

ffl g "

d d d

_ II CO ^ d b£)

Ph

O

fH

o

^ r , CO bJO d H (D r-! ‘H s

H-> p, O CO ^ rQ

c

H ^

d

rH

> b

03 r-H CD

r— H O

h

p

fH

bX) P

fH

Ph

P

CO H->

d

fH

p

rd

H->

■a "

rJP . 4H

XJ P

P fH H-H

d I

>

•rH

H->

U d

d

p p rP

CO

d

rd

CM

<

d

•rH

bX)

d

rH

d

d

p

d

d

p5

X5

d

fH

d

d

d

d

fH P

o

H->

fH

P

O

-H

d

H-H

CO d CO .,H

H

4-)

oJ

fH

P

4H

5

M-H

o

d

fH

a

rH

P CO

fH

rH

H->

CO

O P

Q

d

cS

P

0

P

O rd

rH

d

fH

-4->

in

K

c

W

a

+H

d

p

p

xf

o

U

rH

•rH

X)

o

42

to

CO

o

fH

o

d

H

in

X!

<1

W

p

fi

I

rH

H

Id

after entry into Aj.

Freeze Like a hesitate or stop, but with the body noticeably-

stiffened, often elongated, and completely motionless. Generally no vibrissae movement. The animal often jerks into this characteristic posture.

-M

•tH

>

•iH

o

rt

M

U

•iH

PI

<u

•iH

Pi

0

1

Pi

O

4->

a

(U

o

<u

pci

. PI

2 ® 2

.iH H

- Pi ^

nS 4-> PP CO

d o

o

rd

-M

•iH

4^

o o

d

<D

d

4->

d

o

u

u

cc5

d ^ ^ >> g.«

■s £ .

>> [xj W I— I ^

4-> d

. 0

tJ d p^ d d d <L) <u Ph Cu

^ > o

0)

d

(D

in

O

P

d!

P O O

o > o

p -d w O -H

p

o

o

I— I

<+H

<^-l

H

d

CO

CO

T3 d d 0)

d

CO (U CO T)

rH

CO

I

d

"d

d

d

P’S 4J

•4-i

•d 'd t> (U

o

u

d

p

4->

d

P

P

o

p

o

o

x>

4-> CO P d d

+J -p

d CO

S g

^ S'"

P d >S tv M

M

0) ^

-I '-M

o

o ^

bjo

ci;

d w

(U o

•p ^

p

o ^

p

o o tj

rP

PH §

O 6

bO

d X ^

d rP

(i> a ^

d

a;

,d

CJ

(U

o ^

u ^

(D Sh CJ

CD ^

^ d CO

D -p

•P H-> O

bJO UO bX)

d d d

iH r~t tH ^ m

♦I— I ‘H ‘i-H

d d d

CO CO CO

>S

d

p

o, O

o3

+J TP

<P 'HH <P MH m »p m <p

•r-( "iH *rH ‘iH

d d d d

CO CO CO CO

U Vi

^ U H Q m in in

-p

o

rP!

CO CO

(U 0)

U rd

p u d d o d

CO d 4-)

d

o

^ bJO

p d

o -rt

d

d

d d O iH o ^

•rH

d d

•H CO

0)

4-> >P

O

d o

•iH

-M

o

d

p

d

•p^

o>

CO

r~l

CO

d

0) o O

4->

o ^

CL, d

p '"d o P 'hi cc5

CO ^ CD O

,P 4->

s ^

•S d

d p

•P o

d

CO cri

d iP d

0) -H (U

p d .p

o o

•Xi ^ Ss

d x!

X) o

^ CD d3

*rH

^ d ^

° d P

O a o

(D p

CJ 'd O g P k o

CL, ^ p d

p

CO p4

iH

fU

'+H ^

(D O ^

-

o

cn p O

^ S w

d p bJD

d

CD

p

p

o

CO

d

p

d

p

d

a,

CL,

d

CD

p

p

O

CL,

o

p

o

p

X3

(D

p

d

CD

rH

p

o

T)

d

CD

rd

CO

d

CL,

(D

p

O

P

rd

P

•H

d

(D

4J>

O

t s-

m te

b>s ^

S o

O

X3 "Xj CD

X)

P

d

p

O

P

p

o

d

o

•tH

u

<D

P

•tH

X!

0 P

P

bX)

CD

X

O

O

1

IT)

CO

CD

CO

CO

d

P,

p

d

p

CO

d

d

CD

CD

CO

d

p

p

p

o

p

p

CD

CD

(D

CD

^ h O

[p

P

(D

p

d

CD

o

bfi

d

* rH

p

d

d

d

CL,

d ^

J> (O

l> CD ^ rd

bX)

d

bX) P

Ph

CL,

O

p

d

o

CJ P

u d d (D

pc!

d rd W)

d

•H

CO

p

o

p

rd

bX) .

•H P

?4 ^

O tH P P

CO jp CQ CD

0 U

^ o

bfl p

O O

p rd

pH CL

!>srd

d bX)

^ p ^

p

d

P

Ph

P

d

p

Ph

p P P O P d O u P P X d O [p

bX) rX

Ph £ CO Ph

O'

u

Ph

CL

Ph

Q

Ph

I

d

fp

p

O

p

w

P

p

d

bO

d

rH

X d o

CL CO bX) P

p ^

p o X P

p

rs <U

H 4J

m p P O

•rH i>s

> P

25

d

X

P

X

d p •1—1

p 0) (U

CL -H

a

8 p u

^ hH

O r-H 'H ccj

X ^ (D 5^ P k d o

<u

■§ - o m

p

P o

X P

p p p

O P ■H P

P P

P

P

?H P

CO X fn

P

CO p

p p

0) -rH

P P P

^ -5 ^

d P w

P o d

p - d p >s •H X P

CD H

p o

p

p

p p

X d

p H CJ

: .g

P CO -H

d p >

odd P X d P

a ^

bO

P fS

P P

O ^

J CL

d

S 0^

O

X

CL

a

o

p

26

competing or accessory behavior between the first and last photocells marking off the alley proper, was scored as a "direct run" (DR) trial. A trial on which competing and accessory behavior both occurred was scored simply as a CB trial.

From the video-tape recordings a graph was made of each trial for every subject. On a schematic "map" of the alleyway, each behavior cate- gory which occurred was plotted at the appropriate locus, and it was noted whether the behavior was competing or accessory. Receptor -orienting activities were competing if they occurred while the subject’s rear legs were stationary, and accessory if they occurred while the rear legs were moving forward. The amount of time spent in competing behavior per CB trial was measured from the recordings with a stopwatch. Behavior occurring at a photocell was counted as occurring in the alleyway segment begun by that photocell. Thus if a subject stopped and sniffed the second photocell, thereby activating it, this competing behavior was scored for A^, even if most of the subject's body was still in the latter part of A^. Behaviors in SB prior to a trial, in the 7.0 cm. segment between the start door and A^, and during retraces were recorded on the maps but were not used in classifying trials, or in determining any of the data presented here. It was also noted for each trial whether the subject showed a galloping gait, with both front and both hind legs moving together in unison, or some other gait ("run"). With the present video -recording system, and in the absence of clear guidelines for classifying rat gaits (cf. , Gray, 1968),

T j i . -• r (T

27

finer gait differentiations could not be made reliably.

When there was some doubt on a given trial as to how an observed behavior should be categorized (< 5% of all recorded behaviors), the tape was reviewed and compared with other trials, already analyzed, showing similar behavior. When some doubt remained even after such review (< 15% of all such cases) as to whether a behavior should not be counted or should be counted as accessory behavior (e.g., jerky head-movements while running, abrupt changes in gait), or as to whether a behavior was competing or accessory (e.g., H or AH?), a conservative criterion of behavior other than running was used; decisions between AB and no AB were resolved in favor of no AB, and decisions between CB and AB in favor of AB.

Another observer, with some training in the use of the present clas- sification system, but with no knowledge of the conditions of the experi- ment, independently graphed the first six trials for every subject, and also graphed selected later trials for some subjects. In all cases there was virtually complete agreement as to whether a given observed behavior was competing or accessory (> 95%) and as to precise locus (> 98%), and almost as high a level of agreement as to the specific category (> 90%). Agreement on gait classification (gallop or "run"), based on a sample of 15 trials for each subject, was > 95%. All disagreements were resolved by reexamination of the video-tapes.

Each trial was reviewed on video-tape by the experimenter at least

t n

4.

■»vt

ic .* ...-.■•■♦»■» I

r'„'>*ll ' .'ll 1 t. Fv * i L

t\i ,■ < I , ) ^ l.^-... .

: »?j '40t|.>

^ '*

28

seven times, including a minimum of three times after every other run had been graphed at least in a preliminary way, to ensure uniform appli- cation of the classification system throughout the experiment. Also, each run was reviewed again after every trial for Experiment II had been graphed.

Results

Acquisition

Figure 2 presents a description of alleyway behavior during acquisi- tion up to the first change in conditions (trials 1-25 for the early-shift subjects, trials 1-33 for the others) for each individual in Gp H, and Fig- ure 3 presents similar data for Gp T. For each subject the amount of reward in acquisition is shown. The description in these figures proceeds from a gross level in the top panel to a more molecular level in the lower panels. The top panel for each subject shows the overall run time on each acquisition trial. Run times greater than 25 sec. are plotted at 25 sec. The three types of trials are coded separately with small dots represent- ing CB trials; filled squares, AB trials; and open circles, DR trials. The second and third panels show the duration of competing behavior and the number of bouts of competing behavior on each CB trial. The duration of competing behavior is shown to the nearest second, except that all dura- tions less than 0. 5 sec. are plotted at 1 sec. A bout began when the sub- ject elongated out of SB into A^ or hesitated, froze, or stopped after entry

Fig. 2. Behavior in Acquisition for Gp H.

101 sm. :| I 109 sm ] | lOSmed. ^ Il3med. 108 Ig.

(oas) auu!! un^

ro rvj -

(oas) sinoq ^ ^ ^

uoiiDjnp g^oN AjoSaiDO JoiADgag

0 0

T rials

Fig. 3. Behavior in Acquisition for Gp T.

(oas) auLiu unjj uouDjnp ao 83 ON snaoi puo Ajobaioa joiAopaa

T rials

1

33

into 3-nd ended when the subject's rear legs began moving forward

again. A retrace was scored as a single bout regardless of the number of separate stops and starts which occurred during the retrace. The bottom three panels show for each of the three segments of the alleyway all of the categories of competing behavior which occurred on each CB trial (filled dots). Each CB trial is marked with a short vertical line on the abscissas of each of these last three panels. The order of the categories on the ordinate from bottom to top (a-q) is roughly in order of decreasing fre- quency for both groups combined, throughout the experiment. Crosses near the top of the three panels represent each occurrence of categories not listed, e.g., defecation. Unfilled dots at the top of these last three panels mark the occurrence of accessory behavior in each segment, but not broken down into categories since, as will be shown later, only three categories occurred with notable frequency. The detailed description in the lower panels of these and subsequent figures focuses on competing behavior since changes in accessory behavior throughout training closely paralleled those seen for competing behavior.

It can be seen in the top panels of Figs. 2 and 3 that run time decreased across trials for each subject. The decrease was relatively orderly for some subjects (e.g., rats 101, 109, 113, 108, 110, 106, 111), but for others periods of very long run times occurred after early acqui- sition (see rats 105, 102, 112, 103, 107). For all subjects except 105, 102, 112, and 103, the first few acquisition trials were the longest of all.

*

V ...>

.V'' ' J.ff<yr . ■, '

, vu M .<v rtL.Jiv

' f U ... yJ'/fcs^^JuH ;*i»> *’r-:’'^i

> - se

'.■7H-- r'

^ ^ !if4#J)J8ii IWM*» V|.:. ; iv '■ l''-M>

r,j a.* t ,rv I . fe.- ,^,:

I '.•■■•( ,:,i l"-- ' ••; i

t -5?*W

'■ .' . . &

' t k\

t-4 •jrtili'Sj

i3. <?' -

■* >6' ' ■‘^^‘

, ^ * M i •iTi** <•' •' ' '

w- ^

34

Competing behavior invariably occurred on these long early trials. As succeeding panels of Figs. 2 and 3 show, this competing behavior on the very first trials usually was of long duration, occurred in multiple bouts, involved many categories, and was seen throughout the alleyway; this was particularly the case for the thirsty subjects. Accessory behavior, typi- cally prolonged floor -sniffing, also occurred throughout the alleyway on the Long early acquisition trials for all subjects. The large decrease in overall run time in acquisition reflects a corresponding decrease in the '^amount'' of noninstrumental behavior, which was qualitatively similar for the hungry and thirsty subjects. The behavioral changes underlying the decrease in the amount of competing and accessory behavior are illu- strated for each individual subject in Figs. 2 and 3, and are summarized quantitatively for the two groups across the first 25 trials, in blocks of 5, in Table 2. (Note that in the group averages in Table 2 there is an increase in run time and a corresponding increase in measures of com- peting and accessory behavior on trials 11-15 due to rats 105, 113, 108, 112, 103, and 107 showing longer trials at that point, and a similar increase in some measures for Gp T on trials 21-25 due largely to rats 102 and 107.) First, the frequency of occurrence of CB trials decreased across acquisition. This can be seen for individual subjects in several ways, e.g., the decrease in the frequency of marks along the abscissa of any of the bottom three panels of the figures, or in the frequency of entries in panels 2 and 3, and can be seen overall for the two groups in

« V.

r i.';i V:'

T . ,!/ X^,} 1 <|.t ■<(»,

35

Table 2

Sirmmary of Behavior Changes in Acquisition (Trials 1-25) for Both Groups

Subjects Trials

1-5 6-10 11-15 16-20 21-25 X

Panel #

1 X Run Time /Trial

Gp H

11. 01

5. 05

5.41

3. 35

2. 81

5. 52

Gp T

21. 66

6. 70

11. 92

3. 98

4. 94

9. 84

2

Frequency of CB

Trials

/Block

(CB Run

Time)

Gp H

. 60

.47

. 63

. 30

. 23

.45

(8. 60)

Gp T

. 77

. 60

. 67

.43

.40

. 57

(13. 83)

3

Frequency of Trials with AB/Block

(AB Run

Time)

Gp H

.87

.47

. 57

. 37

. 23

. 50

(3. 52)

Gp T

.93

. 63

,. 63

. 37

. 37

.59

(3.90)

4

Frequency of DR

Trials /Block

(DR Time)

Gp H

. 10

. 33

. 20

. 50

. 57

. 34

(2. 58)

Gp T

. 07

.27

. 20

.40

.40

. 27

(3. 03)

5

Time in

CB (Sec.

)/CB Trial

Gp H

6. 82

3. 33

3.40

1. 53

.90

3. 20

Gp T

16. 35

4. 10

10. 83

1. 09

5. 41

7. 56

6

# Bouts

of CB/CB Trial

Gp H

2.48

1. 32

1. 35

1.40

1. 00

1.51

Gp T

3. 81

1. 75

2. 14

1. 30

1. 33

2. 10

:((**».»i|(*X'.

1 .,,t4tet''

'“■^2

■%

■«3P7

'^1

I

4li iQ;^C4.f%tm'^i-

iqc^WSjafl t

'm

4k»4i

Ai8 : ;*: St-

-S; %

tjj- -,

,Vii'

: tmK

HK^if nA) /S3.tl

Ttiv .1 j

utr .1

•f ' .♦

»

lL<iWk>ii

M ,f

I ^n,iiy ■I'-ii- i^iijiir- |'■llJ.^^, .

. ' 'V:- ebS i'

3 '^:-

. . ^ _. 2' . ' JJ-'- . _K ,, »f. h

sp.c iiT,j

; i

r^y.

dieT •»'X‘ ,‘lc!>' Y1&0 iit(k^'(»^

re.

te.. ,4^',:’

_:-' :iS" ■.■>i-'....

I ^

B.

■, : ' it . '■"

US'. , f^:. .2'T^r:,,

Iiii-I ^V(

: ':■^■v ;■ .^ ' ^''y sn_ s

..(r . . . EP .ot Pli*f J

St-

33:u!^

, 'r' ^ ' •■‘»

'V'*' a.^

■^’ -

4u»r uf^/i ^^:a ?€.r

■Ii«'.»i'', -’, 0t .r,;« m'i niS)

36

Table 2 (continued)

Subjects Trials

1-5

6-10

11-15

16-20

21-25

X

Panel #

7

# Categories of CB/CB Trial

Gp H

5. 30

4. 21

3. 67

3. 37

2.40

3. 79

Gp T

6. 64

4. 27

4. 50

2. 87

4.42

4. 54

8

Proportio:

n of CB

Trials

on Which CB Occurred

in Each

Alleyway Segment

Gp H

. 60

. 33

. 04

. 20

. 20

. 27

^2

. 43

. 17

. 14

0

. 20

. 19

. 75

. 58

.95

1. 00

. 50

. 76

Gp T A,

. 77

. 25

. 34

. 32

0

. 34

.72

. 71

.39

. 20

.47

. 50

.97

.47

. 82

. 58

. 74

. 72

9

Proportion of CB

and AB

Trials

on Which AB Occurred

Each Alleyway Segment

Gp H A,

. 73

. 67

.25

. 25

. 53

.49

^2

. 81

. 56

.40

.39

. 20

.47

.90

. 74

. 80

1. 00

.47

. 78

Gp T

. 78

.64

. 33

.47

. 20

.48

^2

. 88

. 68

.39

. 22

. 25

.48

-1

1. 00

.48

.79

. 72

. 80

. 76

37

the second panel of Table 2. The frequency of occurrence of accessory behavior also decreased in acquisition, as the bottom three panels of the figures and the third panel of Table 2 show. Corresponding to the decrease in the frequency of trials with competing and accessory behavior, the fre- quency of DR trials increased in training (Figs. 2 and 3; Table 2, panel 4).

Not only the frequency, but also the length of CB trials decreased in acquisition, as the top panel of Figs. 2 and 3 shows. This was mainly due to a decrease in the time spent in competing behavior per CB trial, shown in the second panel of the figures and panel 5 of Table 2. The length of AB trials also decreased in acquisition (Figs. 2 and 3, top panel). This decrease similarly reflected a decrease in the time spent in accessory behavior, yielding more unimpeded running per AB trial, though this is not shown. The decrease in the time spent in accessory behavior also accounted for a small part of the decrease in the overall run time on CB trials showing accessory as well as competing behavior.

As the duration of competing behavior decreased, the number of bouts of competing behavior per CB trial decreased (Figs. 2 and 3, panel 3; Table 2, panel 6), the number of categories per CB trial decreased (Figs.

2 and 3, last three panels; Table 2, panel 7), and competing behavior became concentrated in the first alleyway segment, farthest from the goal- box, especially for Gp H (Figs. 2 and 3, last three panels; Table 2, panel 8). Comparable changes occurred in accessory behavior. However, though accessory behavior did become more concentrated in A , it

'It* " ■^=1 - sA'r .H irfbVc) i^U

,< !»■■ '"str

K f Wv

•t #•» r ■%..■

'fWj'aw ,.’soj;:. .

*M Ji

■.V ••■ !■'( <?•; -I, ; .

i, ■• '■' , " ^ 7^ ^

- 4 - o»*lJI lUfiJ

'". r,t!*c £

U-*"|iNt*»VV &>'•***

Ji

isq i^riii)4iiiT(rtili .rf; 111

•i

a* . •'M'iii'

P ...3

M'fjl rj$tt ?n » u4i •n^gsSb

’- '-* -^•■^'* ■?

I) ^ fl ‘-j*!--.,

^ UM-"?

■» ,)«* ''.t Vo

;*>. /tar|;!|.t; :#4. iSRi-v^rti'-ri safe ’yisrvv tifi4"^l

!''•.« 9 ■''.

' i .r,.' crrl-S^i. ^ «*:i JOt'iftTto ■*■ to

r>1 *i'

.> >. -J*,o ^ o T

, ^

t< » 'i vij^^iJcT' 's' 1 '

ft?'

■„ ''^ ■y«¥'''^'/f "to.r.f’ ,^, tot s jiuks^\,. v

■••V :rl ■■■J^i:iflM.||»# ■. '■ ^;»0--;’^<|i'^ !)'»'< ^ *»•=!?*

'xi t«iii4rt

. ^:, -

~‘>iin oexmoM<) hi^f^

38

continued to occur more frequently in A and A farther into acquisition than competing behavior for both groups (Figs. 2 and 3, last three panels; Table 2, panel 9).

Thus, most of the decrease in overall run time in acquisition is clearly attributable to the decrease in the number of trials showing com- peting and accessory behavior, and the decrease in the time spent in com- peting and accessory behavior given such a trial. However, decreases in competing and accessory behavior alone did not account for all of the decrease in overall run time in acquisition. Figures 2 and 3 show that there were also consistent decreases in DR time across trials for every subject of both groups. With very little variation between subjects, the longest DR trial was about one and a half times as long as the shortest in acquisition (trials 1-25 for the early-shift subjects, and 1-33 for the others) and about twice as long as the shortest for the entire experiment. This is shown quantitatively for individual subjects in Table 3. For the individuals of both groups, AB trials were longer than DR trials at any point in training, as Figs. 2 and 3 show, but showed the same ratio of longest to shortest (1. 5 ; 1 in acquisition and 1.9: 1 throughout), with more variation between subjects than for DR trials. CB trials were the longest of all at any point in training, and for each subject showed a vastly larger ratio of longest to shortest, averaging around 10:1 in acquisition and throughout for Gp H and 20 : 1 for Gp T, with large differences between

subjects .

; n ' ♦t.'T j - i

. , <'■ ,ii.

'

-4?1> I .

u*

l> i .4 flt xf* ••’'•' <itt tt»' ;J/ -^AV .•

' ".i . ' '■ ' "‘ ^ '

, ; *rs.'V>< ^ o' ^’)Q «:»y^

folv tit^itd t « i u ^io.4 g>j-<0’9 *$Jtf <>-:> '

I , -i.j, *

^ >*

V . ’I * >*>^ t'-ntl’i

r, 4. ■“ "i^f, 1. ' ,.,’ . 4 1' i-jf ^crj/.t 'i,’X

y»*e.-7 ‘i.u ' .<3Jt .■■ifrii -v‘ fttoVi«aT:3-^h .*^’'

4)ic * .

' ; .:'/:3tf: , «’> '■•*''••■ »i4i^K t'ij'V ■' xiJyvi- ip-

^ -^' ^ -S!

. .,.J ■■jiinit ma |,J^V^4a.'J^.fW"i^» ■<'■ '4^#^ ith^;h§ ^^4^/V'' I

■'■*4- ■■■■ ■;■■■ ..

ITi’ '•

■‘ n * "'.I J

, J .

-j.M 7- »»j =■ ' u,tsbVi£i'4i rni -v '

t' a44kVa,^<-i ' ' 1 ., !-oi ■-•• .'/ 5^.ii::i -»..{

W0:' '

i'7‘ _ " »aV' V

'Ih>^3o

. ^ -■ . .:. :.y m' " "

•■' .1 «(- . - u..r hV/O/* i' ' M«- 5 ,^4'?'5'4g J^joj

' n.'i* j. trii I t l‘*i'tc , tpi4i*A=i^p - j i'li;

> •:. ' / ^ ^

■* I -,»-,;-.f ■1'j “loi

.&<■ I

; . r.'

J- TM

J t.

f,,

_. i” '

..< Twt f‘n-& p)/lUft«Tj 43t i;Ai Yi --si -*5 it^.rs *

^ 7

J t Oi biu ■t '■’ :ji ni^/i-so-/»' pa

'. .fcl*ili% ..,'%".iP*‘»'5i 1 ^OS jtrjtiJK '■^' -ivo4%»^1wii

LT

p»l'

39

Table 3

Range of DR Times in Acquisition and Throughout Training

Subjects

Longest DR Trial (Sec. )

Acq. (Throughout)^

Shortest DR Trial (Sec. )

Acq. (Throughout)

Long

Acq.

Ratio

est/Shortest

(Throughout)

Gp H

101

3. 60

2. 63

(1.70)

1.4

(2.1)

109

3. 04

1. 89

(1.52)

1. 6

(2.0)

105

2. 86

1. 77

(1.49)

1.6

(1.9)

113

3. 14

2. 05

(1.72)

1. 5

(1.8)

108

3. 68

(3. 76)

2.43

(2. 06)

1. 5

(1.8)

no

2. 69

1. 75

(1.58)

1. 5

(1.7)

X

3. 17

(3. 18)

2. 09

(1. 68)

1.5

(1.9)

Gp T

102

3. 23

(4. 00)

2. 20

(1.98)

1.5

(2.0)

112

3. 62

2. 90

(1.92)

1. 3

(1.9)

103

3. 20

2. 06

(2. 02)

1. 6

(1.6)

107

3. 35

2. 32

(1. 88)

1.4

(1.8)

106

2. 97

(3. 16)

2. 30

(1. 80)

1.3

(1.8)

111

4. 85

2. 74

(2. 26)

1.8

(2.1)

X

3. 54

(3. 70)

2.42

(1.98)

1. 5

(1.9)

^No entry in this column indicates that the longest DR trial in acquisition was also the longest throughout training.

i

(■ ' ,1^ . -li I

^S^dn3

,: J

„v.- - ipMMW>|j|dfei^i«aWW^ .'II ' »^r«»>W)WaaPlg%i^Wl^

40

Effects of amount of reward, and deprivation state and associated reward, on acquisition

Despite the small number of subjects at each reward magnitude in acquisition and the considerable quantitative differences between subjects at the same magnitude, subjects receiving large reward generally ran faster in acquisition than those receiving medium or small. This is shown quantitatively for a representative block of acquisition trials, 21-25, in the first column of Table 4. Correspondingly, the large -reward subjects in both groups showed slightly fewer trials with competing and accessory behavior and more DR trials across the first 25 trials, as is shown in the next three columns of Table 4. Also, for Gp H, but not Gp T, the total time spent in competing behavior during acquisition varied inversely with amount of reward. Within Gp H, excluding rat 108, the point before a change in conditions at which stable DR runs began to predominate was reached first by the subject receiving large reward, next by those receiv- ing medium, and last, if at all, by those receiving small, as Table 4 shows. (Note also in Fig. 2 that even though rat 108 (Ig. ) did not show stable DR runs, his overall run time did stabilize rather early, at trial 24.) It was not possible to determine this relationship in any detail for Gp T since only one subject, rat 106, showed stable DR performance before a change in conditions. However, it is consistent with the results for Gp H that this subject was receiving large reward. Finally, it is also seen in Table 4 that, for those rats who showed a stable DR asymptote

~ I,

* 1 ,

,11, »1 1 1 ' ,„»|» K^^wwii- -v^'i I in'miiiiwipijii’

Vfi^ '■" " ' *A

^1 otlqi3«J*a

' fl

! Vv/9'J .1 . 'fe’lt 'iMI « »f £VJ^J5 ..i>|l%iljrrT%4ur|<^l|: «4^ tB.

,»i.

j.T. ’■

'* 'fif aAifh •tMlTefii^!iii.

'

» - J ^

i-tit'% ' vi V»»WSt4f£%ii

' nj..'^!ljv','

. i'i

' ’'■ ii£ . *• ft a 4 '#'iii3

I’TPa^i

li , -j

* *

liiX' 'taWtif' ■<tt4i » ' J'»* fca ^ "*■%

r^i*:

I : ; -It tC’ »'-i

uy £f" ' 1 t'n-;j4 ai

1. •.•.(, ■'.; . n:t‘- ,|1(U

» C fT^ 7i^'A>,lr;;u»,p -t," 'i> #:: ~dJ \\^ . ^

'!?*'' ^ :* i 'li^-l'C.'fT - il'.-C ^ aSWllf«

1/iiS)* . r V 7; ^

■f iitt/Tz f* •■:-■ - J » f . . t^.j rt (t/y . .i-wf: ■>J<i*^ 4"'|

^■: i7/S

^ssr. " ..-4

Iff.

M

, '■ '^'01".-' -,

‘f j^ lA V-svn^’ Jr-i '>r;y. T

'n

•»*.- ..■*ll'J,: ; .!i'77'f0> 5‘'^£i'l''t •®^t04i4^,'X04, trt :.^aj^

i :-j;v<*:.'l/M'y(l 1' ' JsSt<4;J ■«•-*'•■ B ‘«0^

. J .V Jv.it 3 itij»ifeal

'y5>9i*-

41

42

late in training (trials 50-54), run time varied little as a function of reward magnitude. Thus, reward magnitude affected the rate of the acquisition process (mainly the rate of the dropping out of competing and accessory behavior, but also the rate of decrease in DR time), but did not greatly affect the final asymptotic level of performance.

It is clear from the data presented in Figs. 2 and 3 that while the proc.es s of acquisition was qualitatively the same for Gp H and Gp T, it was slower for the thirsty subjects. Overall run time was longer through- out (see Table 4, first column), and every behavioral change underlying the decrease in overall run time in acquisition was slower for Gp T (Table 2, panels 2-9) with the single exception that accessory behavior continued to occur in A and A about equally often for the two groups through the first 25 trials. Thus, the thirsty subjects showed more trials with competing and accessory behavior, fewer DR trials, and their CB, AB, and DR trials were all longer than those of the hungry subjects in acquisition. They showed more time in competing behavior, more bouts of competing behavior, and more categories of competing behavior per CB trial, and a higher frequency of competing behavior outside A^ at every stage of acquisition than the hungry subjects. Finally, in contrast to the hungry subjects, only one thirsty subject (rat 106) showed stable DR per- formance before a change in conditions.

.

.

rr. ^ "-T*

'j- 'ti ■;• ,. «

'4, '*' 4<(f» r»J <

rvVdfi

iiff v' 4..

( / »

;■ rv Jtt'lW, >.l!. •■'*■♦'

v-a . -.1

t,,^/.'iVjCji'v'‘'y^'i

:g®>

fs.-> f 1 3.. y Titiss 1

k9«

MWP?«ii-Uji(5: ■' i 'V:( ''^'''*^k>i•^^y^

#a.

^‘:f .‘>%,

■'< •,' c

t n--' >D =:t.‘ , ’..( ..- (V. is t(il' 4if.fl

t ) -•* ^ ■»« IciU j

. f

1 .4p^

I I

-i

‘A,bviB ■•

'3v^'\v

V r?<iJ -t-i *u , '<

till «*»>■: 1

,i^.<

-Vid.3 a? ■'•

-u.

e5!._ ®

I ; '

i r,!'.^ ii4^aif<!*

..■V, / . ' . ''

r . - 1 -4 ■/) ,4^;^ V *3 fi wif a-n:4 ,4i|0ri ji'J

c hdf^Q ai h

" '' ’■'

Kaitivi ,ywi|>i<>wiw[i;in,a

43

Shifts in amount of reward

Figure 4 shows the performance of individual subjects in Gp H fol- lowing reward magnitude upshifts at early, middle, and late stages of training, plotted as in Figs. 2 and 3. Figure 5 shows comparable data for Gp T. Performance on the last three trials preceding the shift (marked by a long vertical line from the abscissa) is also shown for each subject in both figures. Only those categories of competing behavior which occurred for a group during upshifts are listed on the ordinate of the bot- tom three panels in these figures. It can be seen clearly in the individual data for Gp H that upshifts in amount of food at any stage of training were followed by fast, stable DR runs. Following a preceding downshift, how- ever, it sometimes took several trials before the upshift resulted in stable DR runs (e.g., rats 108 and 113, late upshift). The apparent magnitude of the effect depended on the preshift baseline of behavior. The upshift effect appeared large whenever baseline performance consisted of rela- tively long and variable runs, especially involving competing and accessory behavior, such as early in training (rat 101) and at middle or late stages of training following a preceding downshift (rats 110, 108, 113). The effect appeared very much smaller when preshift runs were already stable, asymptotic DR's, as for rats 109 and 105 at the middle shift. The effects of upshifts in amount of water (Fig. 5) similarly involved decreases in amount of competing and accessory behavior and decreases in DR time. Yet, while run times were faster and less variable for most subjects of

44

Fig. 4. Behavior Following Upshifts in Amount of Reward for Gp H.

ro

<

(\J

<

(D0S)

auuji un^

CO

o

I-

46

t

Fig. 5. Behavior Following Upshifts in Amount of Reward for Gp T.

d>

</)

O

I-

48

Gp T following upshifts, they were not as stable as for Gp H. There were usually fewer DR runs postshift, more residual competing and accessory behavior, and greater trial -to -trial variability overall than with upshifts in amount of food, as comparison of Figs. 4 and 5 shows. Rats 102 and 107 showed no obvious improvement in performance after an upshift, and only rat 112 showed postshift performance as stable as that of the hungry subjects. The apparent magnitude of the effect for Gp T, as for Gp H, generally depended on the rate and stability of preshift run times.

Figure 6 illustrates performance for the individuals of Gp H follow- ing downshifts in amount of food, plotted as in Figs. 4 and 5. Note that performance following the early downshift for rat 110 is plotted through trial 54, while for the other early-shift subjects in Figs. 4-7 performance is shown only through trial 33. The latter subjects either clearly showed a disruption of running by the extra stimulus at trial 34, and their data are therefore presented later (rats 101 and 111), or they showed almost exclu- sively fast DR runs postshift, and are therefore not shown (rat 112). It is seen in Fig. 6 that downshifts in amount of food at early and middle stages of training resulted in a large-scale return of competing and accessory behavior and a lengthening of DR times. At the late stage of training, the subject without previous experience with small reward (rat 105) showed a comparably large effect. However, the subjects with previous experience with small reward showed very small effects, involving a slight lengthening of DR times (rat 110) or an occasional fast CB trial (rat 109). The effects

Fig. 6. Behavior Following Downshifts in Amount of Reward for Gp H.

Early

Midd le

Late

109

Ig.-sm.

3d*0cAA>

20-

10-

4 -

II

0-

m-

I » <»-

I

i

.t

JJL

1 T

70 75

- 105 ] Ig -sm

70 75

Trio Is

51

I

1

Fig. 7. Behavior Following Downshifts in Amount of Reward for Gp T.

I

No.CB

Behavior category and locus bouts CB dur. Run time (sec)

> > > (sec)

Early Middle

Late

Trio I s

53

of downshifts with water, shown in Fig. 7, were similar but usually weaker. Thus, the early downshift subject of Gp T (rat 111) showed no clear decremental effect (only one long CB trial postshift), and only rats 103 and 106 at the middle downshift showed large effects comparable in magnitude to those with food reward reductions. There was a similar effect of previous experience with small reward on the late downshift with water, with experienced subjects showing no decremental effect (rat 102), or only a slight lengthening of DR times (rat 111). However, the decre- mental effect even for the thirsty subject not previously receiving small reward (rat 107) was very slight, less than that for the corresponding hungry subject (rat 105).

Thus, as in acquisition, large changes in run time with food or water reward magnitude shifts were mediated mainly through changes in the amount of competing and accessory behavior, though DR time also changed, within a much narrower range. When competing and accessory behavior had dropped out and run time was relatively stable at middle or late stages of training, upshift effects still occurred, but appeared very slight in terms of change from baseline; large downshift effects still occurred, especially for subjects who had not previously experienced small reward. Though there was some overlap among individuals, the hungry subjects as a group showed greater disruption of running with downshifts and more stable performance following upshifts than did the thirsty subjects.

In general, behavior on long trials following reward magnitude

■T

■Vi'

•UMi tie*

ft:. ^ Jl » •■■ I »i#‘!* '■ ♦■ tern

n

:t I

tt4

. w«>ip ' i Hpp:> *»'di #:«u»if!*‘ - 1 ,

4:

iiif ti|'>l^S!|'-&0'I

' 'iJ •' -'ff: iS/

.;' , .'jj= ■' '• ' ' " -9

tfSf^' It.' 4 .•'. .it, tiwA ;7,ISA<^<iTi'4l v:fts riU;W'’l?'^

«' .., ' . ■%'

- ,:|i ^

-.»'>■ WIT; .iM i-fim-ii^,i0^ it if*

I'.-i,.. . ♦■■• ■»♦■-■ ..i3(r»^jdi44i v-inti/l'i

% ii "I ■■,. I,

' •_> ' ' . , , ' ' .-.«• 'mw. ,V:

t- .‘0 !ii

!i I ‘.- .M,> j.uj-r •.; :: i^;f^d3:-,s?|^*rcl -a#-

.:.s .::^>i.ja,

.i.f't : I

iuni igr

4 TV

» #*

■‘j. »* . assMK! ,:.;^':..a;F^

*• ; i<< ' >h tto il ytaaMy/»oxS'

j . w ) r 'jrfi pjfif ii.i,' , ' .1 •:> V i J aJd ■■ B/jS ■a'

•• r^* t!i£*w{« ♦•'fl line. ; ••■••*

. »♦ t

1 ' 1

iTT vii«ff2>aq«^

I <*

Hl':

,SSi ' 3i-

. •«" ( .^I#r \i0tTiil7 -le^a

. •rv-iw'S'; ■(,■ . ■.r:V-J}r'f.-^..

.'I’M

54

downshifts was typical of that earlier in acquisition. It can be seen in Figs. 6 and 7 that, when downshifts engendered very long run times at any stage of training, long -duration competing behavior occurred in multiple bouts, or involved long retraces (m), comprised multiple categories, and was not always confined to early parts of the alleyway (see rats 110, 108, 113, 105, 106, 103). Accessory behavior also often occurred throughout the alleyway on such long postdownshift trials. When run times were shorter, as when the initial effect was less disruptive (rats 109 late and 112 middle), or after a number of trials with the reduced reward (rats 110 early, 113 middle, 101 middle, and 105 late), the trials resembled those later in acquisition. AB and DR trials, often longer than preshift, were seen in addition to shorter CB trials. The CB trials which did occur gen- erally involved a single bout with few categories and, especially for Gp H, seldom outside of A^.

Some individual rats showed categories of competing behavior fol- lowing downshifts which they had not shown earlier in training. Rats 101, 108, and 103 showed turns (n), sometimes with retraces (m), for 1-3 trials; rats 113 and 103 showed freezing (o) for 1-4 trials; rat 110 showed rearing and sniffing the top (p, j) for one trial; and rats 109 and 103 showed rearing in the center of the alleyway (q) for one trial. New categories of accessory behavior also occurred following downshifts, viz,, sniffing the wall on one trial for rat 111 and accessory hesitating on six trials for rat 110. Therefore, the return of competing and accessory behavior with

htt

\r i

>- ’‘**A vf'i

-v.'mf.i}asFa^

t '.‘U <ti '?a7At:t!9ife‘.; fc,^v -to 'tWfllOsrf

. T -* ’ri> tftHMft}

it. 'tj; T,^.'."} ■.' •;

Ir. 3oiW^4J?«r

' mi . lie'll '7- -aii- '-^^''. ■!'' -'^am, '■ tl ! '

f <''• ’• o * it

■ft -

. > i .<%.vfi ■■. -.-■ rt:r>.'/ ?T'3-fj4 t. i

' ^ '■ - ^

tuti *.pj tl;iiil ^**U’ ‘rOUV'MJys. |>

'■ . 7'i ,.v ‘i» ;t«4*40 ' TO ^'jfi.-, 0’A:

'’ ^ ■*"' ,. iiP<P '.^K.

T«. •■«;■ V,-

If®' ' ••

I

:^Ar^Wm

■"" .. ' ■';■ 'S-'-i J' ■' ... ' ,,7 ..i

" ' •' -V. g* , ,,‘t’ ^

..'/ •» •^fcT'*T-rr rr "t».» It *' "'

Hit *i ' '

"im.

a- ' '

■'id 1 A (i'r' a;ij:7 Id.

'-?■ <€.. hS^Vi •]

.. , lAltV- rrrol) v8f*.:f3'^

* •> '..-'i* -'• .'4I. jac ■Kfifi 'ti' i;,-*'^5,.,'#<}'': '4^ Itew *^'

ew 'o.T... hja^ §rj(ia'fH;jj(ijiaa

•V... .52:. ••■T^ r,

55

downshifts was not a simple within-subjects "regression" to earlier alley- way behavior patterns. However, in some cases these "new" behaviors for a given subject had been shown earlier in acquisition by that subject, but occurred either in SB before a trial or during a retrace, and were thus not scored. At any rate, all of these categories were shown in the alleyway earlier in acquisition by some subjects of both groups (Figs. 2 and 3). Thus while downshifts in reward magnitude did occasion the first occurrence of a few categories of competing and accessory behavior for several subjects, there were no categories specific to downshifts. Also, there was no large systematic effect of downshifts on the overall relative frequency of most categories for the two groups; behaviors which were the most common in the early stages of acquisition were also the most common after a downshift, as is suggested by the predominance of points in the lower part of the bottom three panels in Figs. 6 and 7.

"Extra stimulus"

Immediately following the session terminating with trial 33, portions of the floor and ceiling of the experimental room were sprayed with insecticide. Approximately 20 hours intervened between the spraying and the beginning of the next session at trial 34, at which time the odor of the spray was barely detectable to the human observer, and was at any rate quickly adapted to. Nonetheless, several of the subjects showed some disruption of running for several days following the inadvertent introduction

- ^ ?%wi:^\f^ftMfi^ :?

If** •*

I •.»

»

V'.j'f ' --•l'<«<1 jt^H^X<49d Y**VT'

-.r

a gf

V ' g; ', '-rm

:.^d ^* k)r- v‘! 4I

KCl ' *' V '— .■‘-'n *

■'•' ^idb,'dE->lc.'v^? ■'■' w.iH4‘s'^7vrob ^

.. _^^i^

r w+ hMi. io &a ^ -€

'"3

f.v»».c . Mia ot-5 w.

= ■v^ji U ■... ' 1 »** ) 3 ji' ri£h^^J|'i8 J^iol ‘4 X *' f

' -•■ ■■ ''',® ... ' ' '

■.,'.--i*..JU ,/L’ ••..')

T;<W :v r

•■' '■ ■„' ■' k *^mi

' ),

r, .\T^ £i

;lt

j'-m

^

. jT

.i’> .Ur

s -.1 f»««i 4#f t t:,4 "1 .T'fwunjcii *C, ,,

|-»Ciki . '♦■•

-♦j --..'I Lv

•♦ ' J* .■« J

't 3T4 ii \:iit ;’‘ '- 'f

;3t^

» k dvi» ti ,\r Xfii4ll Jq «f)-{ .

.!-■- l-A.

i t- '. :.,- fU "'if. ,;:» >.'i/i;;il‘'^:?^'iy%.^'y-i-'w,e

it»i":r '.iia sq,. ..a»v*'-:. .■■'?. ;4;|,?.ii> *> it* 'XI' »13S

" ,f'<

56

of the extra stimulus. For these subjects there was an increase in the trial -to -trial variability of run time, reflecting the occurrence of long CB or AB trials, and in some cases longer DR trials. Figure 8 shows per- formance under the influence of the extra stimulus, and on the preceding session, for those subjects who showed more than one long CB trial, or a clear increase in the variability of overall run time, or both. It is seen that more subjects in Gp T than in Gp H showed such an effect, and that overall run time was longer, variability in run time from trial to trial was greater, and every measure of amount of competing and accessory behavior was greater for Gp T than Gp H in the presence of the extra stim- ulus .

For both groups the magnitude of the disruptive effect generally decreased across trials and, as with reward magnitude downshifts, the behavioral changes underlying the decrease in overall run time paralleled those seen in acquisition. Thus there was a decrease in the frequency of trials with competing behavior and decreases in the time spent in compet- ing behavior per CB trial, the number of bouts and categories per trial, and the frequency of occurrence of competing behavior outside A^, and parallel decreases in accessory behavior (see especially rats 105, 103,

107, and 106 in Fig. 8).

As with downshifts, some "new" behaviors appeared for some sub- jects following the introduction of the insecticide stimulus (freezing [o] for rats 101, 108, and 1 1 1 on one trial, and accessory facing-right or

Fig. 8. Behavior in the Presence of the Extra Stimulus

for Gp H and Gp T.

Group H Group T

-8

^ ^ -

t ,t

S

B »

»

»

* »

S »

= '

1 1 I 1 m n 1 1 1 ! ’-T' 1 1 1 r“

~f*T n

1 ' IM ' 1 ' 1 M ' 1 i| '1 ' 1 M ' 1 ' 1 M 1 Ml ' 1 '1 '

I'lM'MMMMI'IM

jnp ao

Trials

4*

r

1 -

I t

.i

•f(K ' r»T»i*<'^p!

. , •r‘>’'’rjpY* '

f

t-

t >

^ 'T*ii I 'T

1

f'M ,:1 O' :j .f'‘

' '■ .. A.

59

-left for rat 103 on one trial). But again, these behaviors were some- times shown by these subjects earlier in acquisition in SB before a run, or during a retrace, and were at any rate characteristic of the subjects of both groups earlier in training, and not responses unique to the stimulus change. Also, for both groups the relative frequency of most of the cate- gories of competing and accessory behavior was not systematically affected by the insecticide stimulus.

Locus of competing and accessory behavior and the goal gradient

It was shown earlier that in acquisition competing and accessory behavior dropped out of the latter parts of the alleyway and became pro- gressively concentrated in especially for Gp H (Figs. 2 and 3 and Table 2). Data presented in Figs. 4-8 suggest that these behaviors con- tinued to occur most frequently in A^ with reward magnitude shifts or the extra stimulus. Table 5 summarizes for each subject and each group the proportion of the CB and AB trials throughout the experiment on which competing and accessory behavior occurred in each of the three segments of the alleyway. For each subject competing behavior occurred far more frequently overall in the first segment than in either of the other two seg- ments. While the relative frequency of competing behavior in A^ was about the same for Gp H and Gp T, the thirsty subjects showed somewhat higher relative frequencies of competing behavior in A^ and A^. Acces- sory behavior also occurred most often in A^^ for all but rats 101 and 107,

’Miies tcM ',

C:

-|

,/9

■fi

■■ i-y?c .

!■ >H v=&«i * t -iu. J’lMi ^ it.caxi

, . , .. ' -^s3

^ TO ^

fpilfMIfU ...!4» I^r3i7'v4list4

»»U '■•■ Uiinfi j|*cn-l tjfii

' m''

'S^J

B-:" .vl'^

II

1>u..*Jt^

ejjo'ojyp

ivr

i »

v.:.:**aus^- . ...€'■ J

*«.T^

SO* It

rr*

>-

1)

■» . -o . gf’. ‘***T7

I -ity ., A iv^

■■".• . 1 -■■■‘- :i:. " m. <'..

J I' I

«i

■IT-l^.

,'i u

' . ,'K<(

1 < J •■■■ ■>• ittv# B* f^. ♦* 1 %^d ■'^JWsT

^ f..

4\^- ir li^,. ui

'

'1 i T J -f- fU it.t> j»|Fi ‘tj H Ajbjfi c ' *

I « 7

itl f4y..U^mi^':> 15- {,<.!«/.= ^di

^ ■'^'' ■' 1 " ' sti t’’-

m

r -

^ •; <i' .:').-l- j!0.

;y_

•:f i :; r^d <* .■ ; i :• * i/? : - , T' 4| D S^^i: (fO i

ij’W

!1

:ij

. ,»V bn«i »,TWPs^a^;t' .^la.quJOD r^dsfd^

. . , ■■' '^ --

/*pt. **,-!/ ♦/. ^./iii' iii<lvd^^ T o,|. *i;|ip>^^v.^3j-p

I

I -

^S^.-'i".. T. .-.

Table 5

Proportion of Total CB and AB Trials on Which Competing and Accessory Behavior Occurred in Each Alleyway Segment in Experiment I

Subjects

^1

CB

(AB)

CB

^2

(AB)

CB

^3

(AB)

Gp H

101

. 69

(.-71)

. 46

(.82)

.46

(.65)

109

.94

(.75)

. 18

(.44)

. 06

(.31)

105

.92

(.89)

. 08

(.21)

. 12

(.37)

113

. 74

(.52)

. 16

(.43)

. 37

(.61)

108

. 72

(.71)

. 38

(.53)

. 16

(.45)

110

.92

(.88)

. 20

(.36)

. 04

(.28)

X

.82

(.74)

. 27

(.47)

. 20

(.45)

Gp T

102

. 83

(.83)

.46

(.50)

.29

(.50)

112

. 76

(.86)

. 24

(.68)

. 18

(.27)

103

. 72

(.61)

. 34

(.57)

.25

(.43)

107

. 87

(.65)

. 37

(.69)

.25

(.38)

106

. 67

(.68)

.47

(.45)

.43

(.55)

111

. 82

(.77)

.41

(.55)

.41

(.59)

X

.78

(.73)

. 38

(.57)

. 30

(.45)

61

but for every subject accessory behavior was more evenly spread out in the alleyway than was competing behavior.

Although time-consuming competing and accessory behaviors did occur more often throughout the experiment in than in latter parts of the alleyway, the goal gradient was not entirely due to the early-segment bias of these behaviors. Table 6 shows for each subject and each group the mean run time in each alleyway segment across all trials, and sepa- rately for DR trials, throughout training. Also summarized is the pro- portion of trials showing a positive goal gradient, as indicated by a longer run time in A^ than in A^. Each subject of Gp H showed a much longer mean overall run time in A than in A and showed this pattern on a large

X U

proportion of trials. Three of the hungry subjects showed a "downturn''

in the overall gradient, i. e. , longer run time in A than in A , but not

o c*

longer than in A^^ (rats 101, 105, and 113). Each subject of Gp H also

showed a positive goal gradient on the great majority of his DR trials,

although the magnitude of the A -A difference was greatly reduced by the

X Cl

elimination of competing and accessory behavior. A downturn in the DR gradient occurred for every hungry subject, and for rat 113 A^ time was the longest of all.

A positive goal gradient was also seen on the average for Gp T, both across all trials and on DR trials alone. As for Gp H, the magnitude of

the A -A difference was reduced by the elimination of competing and

X M

accessory behavior. Both overall and on DR trials alone the goal gradient

i*> •h*

i, .i

li»i T ij » , tr."^ •'

. r-

i?.J

-wvix' . ' ;( .:« <rfctcio EW aii;is£^si^‘*^'i'»li«

:t' r^i<i 'ai-;>rt3*j;rt#t4^

tiTK^iTsygao

; -r

fc

r.', , /{ f„ 'i -.i V.,<;‘t..^‘( I* «i9«iJ*

' '• *' r>.l''" --v^.

Tf

•truf.,£lj»s .?nimg|^i4 4yoi-l|

' '.s - . •<. '^ "^■j3>"=’-’''; ■' ■■'■’■

C ^

' If: .-i &,;)■•> «

:l il<! '»loi ^

-'■if

•i^

> vj

r"

c-., ij.ty/iiifAioiU hi\^ „A iJ :AjcjI |

S ' . T - ¥ ' ■' ^

I'^fy '»■ .. ' ■’ ^■-. '4

'••ri- . -**^ . ..’ ^-JpiapUMB

, A mtj' .A al nm

* -> c \ ' .ffl ,' 4 ^ =*lii

it' '.’ - ..'-ri,. .'^',

♦• -Tt- \> '2»A!r't'e HOi-3< .{/•U ltfllfi. .?0i :fMi,rr|:%^i*oi ,

■'

! ?.'■ ir. vl^6tllq|g|cr.'r<j. «lfi.tib' ^

«-■- " '^i

•.'13-->|' Ti-vV r-T9jH-i&''4^r‘ A -- 'iJ lo

:i /■„... rdi-‘-fincip to: ©ji'a. gtji

K'V ■^' ££( ' V' “i ■'

K\^ ^h- M^axu>tf .^::

^■' 'VC 3H^ V

. ■‘' ■■■■■. \ jm ,..U*» '^©iy»^r ditt. r '■

.1 --y

■fe._. iL.- ''ll;:

►f ' ii -

»v

a ■*•*'-' ♦*T!|t;r Jii^j rtpU.^tu ^ iiiroiih^t'i tifaw j A s*iii*ST|^

1*'^ teUfifllttf 'fiA« ' ^ W**l *lp

" ' i -

;'f :--M^

■‘ .; to’j

Ytp9r»:S3&45. ^'I'l

62

Table 6

Mean Run Time (Sec. ) per Alleyway Segment in Experiment I

Subjects Aj A2 Proportion

Ai > A2

Overall

(DR)

Overall

(DR)

Overall

(DR)

Overall

(DR)

Gp H

101

1. 97

(.80)

. 80

(.69)

1. 16

(.79)

. 81

(.89)

109

1. 62

(.75)

. 77

(.59)

. 73

(.65)

.91

(.96)

105

2. 33

(.70)

. 71

(.55)

1. 08

(.69)

.99

(.98)

113

1. 80

(.77)

. 91

(.69)

1. 13

(.79)

. 73

(.74)

108

2.41

(1.04)

1. 35

(.86)

1. 22

(.92)

.83

(.96)

no

1. 45

(.72)

. 77

(.58)

. 75

(. 64)

.96

(.97)

X

1. 93

(.80)

.89

(. 66)

1.01

(.75)

.87

(.92)

Gp T

102

1.89

(.77)

1.66

(.83)

1. 18

(.92)

.47

(.32)

112

1. 95

(.89)

1. 06

(.78)

.93

(.81)

.91

(.94)

103

2. 34

(.88)

1. 67

(.78)

1. 37

(.84)

. 77

(.81)

107

4. 80

(.87)

3. 06

(.80)

1.81

(.81)

. 77

(.84)

106

1 . 66

(.77)

1. 62

(.74)

1. 97

(.88)

. 61

{. 66)

111

2. 17

(.98)

2. 54

(1. 08)

1. 46

(1. 11)

. 37

(.20)

X

2.47

(.86)

1. 94

(.84)

1.45

(.90)

. 65

(.63)

fi-H

j

.M j

ri>t^ ,i -

m

1

"j? " ir ..

'■5 »• .1 ST « *) !'.■*■■

' f«9.li'i

/’

1

■' T .' ‘i& r- ■. r*

1

- '>!i

1

fi.l

iV.

-A.

5S„Jt .^8

■B

a¥^

' i » . f . .

iO'.r

ySCiS

I»«.i, ®4|

“m

H'V.,)

-1 '^'

-i.

0U

{M J:

^ \Jit "'I

■'.S I'

'<' 'tM

\U .1 Vf .

IP.

) ai .1 ( fa ..';

(fg.)

i VT .)

BJf-.

.' *' »l

Tv^X

1 ’m:} -V^ j:' '

« fn . \ .

lij.)

1

4^*1

1

<■.. i

l^.i.' Id -

i<’'^ »)..

T <> J

•V

(j^ .: )i i si .1'^'

(ffv>

’'<» 'V

ttfc*) :♦

m n

i . ?4i . ! < -' <

'^'"sK '

tor-’ ?*‘-:t;* 's<

29

63

for Gp T was seen on fewer trials and was smaller on the average than for Gp H. There were wide individual differences in the goal gradient among the subjects of Gp T. Some thirsty rats (112, 103, 107) showed large overall and DR gradients, comparable to those seen for the hungry rats. However, rat 106 showed only very small positive gradients in both measures, rat 111 failed to show a positive gradient on the average with either measure, and rat 102 did not show the pattern on DR trials. One thirsty subject (rat 106) showed a downturn in the gradient of overall run time, and all showed a downturn in the DR gradient. Further, three sub- jects of Gp T showed the longest times of all on DR trials in (rats 102, 106, and 111).

Gait

It was of interest to determine whether variations in DR time corre- sponded exactly to variations in gait. This could be done only in a general way since only gross distinctions, between galloping and other gaits (”runs'0» could be made reliably. Table 7 shows for each subject the range of run times on DR trials showing each type of gait. Only DR trials showing a given gait for more than two -thirds the length of the alleyway were included. It is seen that for each subject who showed both gallops and '^runs" DR times were generally shorter on gallop trials than on ”run” trials. (Note also that Gp H galloped more than Gp T.) However, for all except rat 107 the fastest "rion" was somewhat faster than the slowest

3M40* t d * ** * * 7, ‘*t\ i I

E- ' i' ^ ' I

1

T «0^1f ’>,■5

.1

.

'. I

««>, App*P|n^' t ifp |io1r -

t

'■T?

» 'I I (■ o'* '■ "blw »' ''w ' ' -Jj tjO tol

t *' , . ■■ 'f. v^:

o«. ■"'. -•:

villi'. )b| it,

" -aaL:—

Iw

V ,. -/ti^i.;, fc' v'M-. -U1 f

■-*.»■ -V(t3'tE^ n»‘

r-Tt.-jM^Af.b -.A b' '.'maM» fflul- ••|

/*‘^»hA'’ jS ^^1:74 JJ% a' ..

.e. . - SM .*.1J rti O Av'..J/*V?.p!a »

* ,.,.■ . fv

I:.a::

^ - 1

\ , . ^V , ■■' -.,>., -s4’

r^. .

r' T ,. *iti*^ (Jfc. i i i&l T^sr-r of ..^ tt

i

i 'Arv/t.-..! „- i,A

> ;/* ' f.)t T . V irf -,.? < ■:> i v&iigin":

jsi rf

rl

'Mil < Kl<:

i-'t '.'

rr.r<; 07pps^l(i1fe4^*^ 'igi "*■■'■«

; 1*. ■-;'> lu.i jivrtJt ja.'iL-»« ,$ji

m

.. ,;

.::i

•'... ' * 4 ' -■

- 'r '^-j'-'fT. i H -/D

m

■'■vn-:‘A > t. r. ^ . » aI'a

64

Table 7

Range of Run Times (Sec. ) on DR Trials

Showing

"Run" and Gallop Gaits

"Run"

Gallop

Subjects

#

Trials

Longest

DR

Shortest

DR

#

Trials

Longest

DR

Shortest

DR

Gp H

101

(39)

3. 60

1. 83

(5)

2. 13

1. 70

109

(29)

3. 04

1. 72

(18)

1. 84

1. 52

105

(10)

2. 86

1. 79

(29)

2. 16

1.49

113

(41)

3. 14

1. 85

(1)

1. 89

1. 89

108

(23)

3. 76

2. 06

-

-

-

no

(1)

2. 01

2. 01

(30)

2. 13

1. 58

Gp T

102

(20)

4. 04

2. 11

(17)

2. 77

1. 98

112

(47)

3. 62

1. 92

(1)

2. 02

2. 02

103

(34)

3. 20

2. 02

-

-

-

107

(17)

3. 35

2. 12

(2)

2. 01

1. 88

106

(30)

3. 16

1. 93

(5)

2. 13

1. 80

111

(46)

4. 85

2. 32

-

-

-

gallop. While gait changes may account then for much of the variation in DR time, run times on trials showing different gaits still overlapped extensively. There was thus no precise correspondence between DR run time and gait.

Range and frequency of competing and accessory behaviors

Table 8 lists the categories of competing behavior seen in the alley- way and summarizes for each subject and each group the proportion of CB trials throughout training on which a given category was observed. The categories of gross movement and receptor-orienting competing behavior are listed in the approximate order of frequency throughout training for both groups combined. Categories which occurred only after the acquisi- tion phase of training was completed are separately marked. For both groups, by far the most common categories of gross movement competing behavior throughout were stopping and hesitating, and the most common receptor-orienting behaviors were sniffing the floor and walls. The rank- order of the other categories was also similar for both groups. This was the case in every stage of training. Next it may be noted that the subjects of Gp H and Gp T did not differ in the range of categories of competing behavior shown in acquisition or throughout the rest of the experiment. There were no categories shown exclusively by one group, and thus no evi- dence for qualitative differences in the categories of competing behavior shown by the hungry and thirsty subjects.

Table 8

Proportion of CB Trials on Which Each CB Category Occurred in Experiment I

Subjects

Catego

ries of Competing Behavior

# CB

Gross Movement

Trials

H

E T 1/2 T 1/4

F

ret

Gp H

101

13

.69

.46

00

CO

. 38

. 08^

. 08^

109

17

. 76

.29

. 18

. 06

0

0

105

25

. 72

. 20

. 24

. 16

. 12

. 04

113

19

. 58

. 26

. 26

. 11^

0

. 21^

108

32

.47

. 66

. 09

. 09^

. 03^

. 03^

no

25

.40

00

. 32

0

. 12

0

X

21. 8

. 60

.39

. 25

. 13

. 06

. 06

Gp T 102

24

. 63

. 63

. 21

. 08

0

. 04

112

17

. 35

. 53

.24

0

. 06

0

103

32

.69

. 22

. 28

. 06^

. 03^

. 03^

107

52

. 54

.46

.48

. 08

. 08

. 08

106

30

.73

. 53

. 36

. 07

. 03

0

111

17

. 53

. 71

.29

. 12

0

. 12^

X

28. 7

. 58

. 51

. 31

. 07

. 03

. 05

a

Category seen only after the acquisition phase of training.

Receptor -Orienting Activity

S(F)

S(W)

S(C)

S(P)

R(W)

S(DW)

S(T)

Fa (r, 1)

R

(DW)

R(T)

R(C)

.69

. 62

. 38

. 15

.23

. 31

. 15

. 08

. 15

0

. 08

. 71

.59

.47

. 06

. 18

. 18

. 12

. 06

. 12

0

. 06^

.64

.44

. 20

. 12

.28

. 24

. 08

. 20

.24

. 08

0

. 68

. 37

. 26

. 11

. 11

. 11

. 05

. 32

. 16

0

0

.78

.38

. 06

. 16

. 06

. 03

.09

. 28

0

0

0 i

.44

.52

0

.24

. 20

. 24

. 04^

. 08

. 16

. 04^

0

. 66

.49

.23

. 14

. 18

. 19

.09

. 17

. 14

. 02

. 02

.75

.71

.42

.21

. 38

. 08

.50

.29

. 04

. 08

0

.88

.41

. 06

.29

. 12

. 18

. 18

. 12

. 18

0

0

. 63

.53

. 13

, 50

. 31

. 13

. 13

. 13

. 13

. 06

. 03^

.54

. 35

. 33

.54

. 19

. 12

. 23

. 21

. 10

. 06

. 04

.53

.47

.27

.23

. 13

.23

. 37

. 13

. 17

. 03

0

.76

.47

.59

.29

.29

.

.24

. 12

.24

.24

. 12

. 68

.49

. 30

.34

.24

. 17

. 28

. 17

. 14

. 08

. 03

68

As Table 8 shows, despite wide individual differences, there was close agreement between the two groups not only in the rank-order of the categories, but also in the mean relative frequency of most of the cate- gories of competing behavior » The main exceptions were S(P) and S(T), which occurred relatively more often for Gp T than Gp H. Thus the evi- dence even for a minimal quantitative difference in categories of compet- ing behavior for the hungry and thirsty subjects was not great.

Table 9 lists the categories of gross movement and receptor - orienting accessory behavior, again in approximate rank-order for the two groups overall, and shows the proportion of CB and AB trials throughout training on which each category occurred for each subject and each group. For both groups, sniffing the floor and accessory hesitations were by far the most commonly seen accessory behaviors at every stage of training, with sniffing the center occurring somewhat less often. No other acces- sory behaviors occurred with notable frequency. There was no strong evidence for qualitative differences in the categories of accessory behavior shown by the two groups. Although Gp T showed two categories not seen for Gp H, viz., R(T) and R(W), both occurred only once, for one rat (107). Also, it is important to note that these behaviors were shown in competing form by the subjects of Gp H as well as Gp T (Table 8). Indeed, no cate- gories of accessory behavior were observed which were not also seen in competing behavior form; this result supports the conclusion, suggested by the observed parallel changes in competing and accessory behavior

isj*.*. i»^» .<M-'i »">.,♦ I*** ^WIOI •■v'l^if'i 4«* li^

'S9»^j-

;c ,

* V : ■;

>w|T 'OlV

%k»J» $v4 * tfi>Hogafi^«D- - p

, k: ...

to,

.■Vf

' '■■■.„ ■'.‘ "^'^aWl^.; ■■ h-' ' ^ i8B^’

" .Hi '•■•

.fur. IV ^yn3i*tM- -j'/Jr. !i ■-i?£’ m Ji ^jt

\Z'm

,v .(

»»<

?.II i. ,'i .1

-<♦'■■!>■-,? < r A. t‘ .■> » . Q'O: la: 4^i'! Jpf-'. : ' ; *^ "-W'«^

f 1 .1 t ' > . I l‘y I f f> ,

> I

' ''y83*''’^?5^a.

' tf

.' - ^ ! ,, - '4^m. ^

,■ i*vo ^-aa; > *d

■I . r

.j:-_ .of Jv'5''<.iv.nxjioa Rsl^\ 'ij;>aQ'

'■ t. i _/i". 1

ft', xipyd^t «?(d.ClO« .'■ 1'^' ^$4‘rir-U3H?

.VJ .V, . . 0-r .^t.?£' an^rfs^,

-av,- •' 'S'Ot'f'jpUt ' •■-•vsf.oiii Jjs^fcl '

. ! t !(?»■ rt .1, iiO i*:iO^»j(r *.' ,H *T,0 t-T Y^'fOToi"'

M

"■1

"■ ' ' ^

s*ot «o,a u pc^ tfUiiiys i-^Jlp*r^ ffaV \<i

.^-' '>. .* ,gr

■' ’Ek

7*1

j .‘iV '■• - '.. ^^■•'■■'4^

rQ

H

a

W

■xJ

<u

0

o

o‘

O

u

o

bO

<D

-(-»

rt

U

m

o

a

H

rC!

o

fl

o

VI

rH

0}

H

H

pq

C

T3

rt

CQ

U

o

H

-l-»

O

&,

0

01

u

o

rH > rt

0)

m

?-i

o

CO

CO

0)

o

o

<

m

o

CO

o;

•H

o

bJO

<u

-l->

ccJ

U

4-»

0

bjo

•rH

4->

fl

•iH

O

1

u

o

-1-1

ft

CD

O

<D

CO

r— !

pq

=«= ^

CO

-M

u

<u

•r-1

Xi

P3

CO

P£j

H

Pi^

U

Ph

H

CO

ft

CO

(c5

ft

CO

U

CO

CO

CO '

S >

^ o (U

o s a

ft

C

o o o o o o o

o o o o o o o

xD vO ro

o o o o o o o

x£) 1—1

o o o o o o o

xO "^00 C^

o o o o o o o

xO CTx I— I CO IT)

O' I 1— I O (xj (XJ I I

xO i-c CO fx- O

f— < I— C (\3 1^

ox xO f-l <Jx C30 CO

CM CO fxj O rH o (XJ

CJ xD CX) ox (XJ

00 LO CX) xD xO fx-

m (XJ cjx CO CO CO

xo CO CO tn (XJ Tt<

fx- xO O CO 00 xO r— H 1 I rH (Xj CO (XJ

CO

CO

(XJ

I— I

o o o o o o o

i-H

o o o o o o o

^ rH

o o o o o o o

00 tn CO

o o o o o o o

00 in (XJ

o o o o o o o

g3

CTX (XJ

o o o o o o o

[X- Ox (jx (XJ in o o

I-H ^0 CO rH r— H

1— I 00 CO cx) m CO xD

fXj t-H 1-H CO x^ (XJ (XJ

(OX rH (x- (JX 00 in (XJ

fx- Ox CX) xO xO O 00

69

xo in (XJ 00 m o o

x;!^ in in xf

x^( CO CO xO (XJ CO

CO CO (XJ CO (XJ (XJ

CO

CO

CO

bo

c!

cc5

!h

-l->

<4H

o

QJ

CQ

cc5

ft

p

O

0

ft

o

ct

d)

rCj

-l->

<D

4h

(ti

s

o

d

(U

(U

CQ

ft

o

bJO

(U

-t->

ccJ

u

ft

ft

o

rH o in CO 00 o

O O O rH O rH

tX!

ft

o

(XJ CO CO xO

O rH O O O

70

throughout training, that competing and accessory forms of a given behav- ior differ from each other only quantitatively, that accessory behavior is in fact simply "weak" competing behavior. The rank-order and the rela- tive frequency of the categories of accessory behavior were comparable for the two groups, except that Gp H showed facing -right or -left some- what more often than Gp T.

Finally, two types of "sequencing" of competing and accessory behavior patterns across trials were observed on a small proportion of CB and AB trials. First, some individuals of both groups developed idio- syncratic behavior patterns that lasted from a few trials to several ses- sions. The first column of Table 10 shows for each subject and each group the proportion of CB and AB trials throughout training on which clear instances of such intr a -individual sequencing occurred. These sequences differed from subject to subject, but almost always involved sniffing some part of the alleyway, e. g. , sniffing the top and wall in A^

(rat 106, trials 5, 7-12), or elongating out of SB, or hesitating or stop- ping, and sniffing the photocell in A^^ (rat 107, 39 trials throughout train- ing). They often occurred in conjunction with other competing and acces- sory behavior not involving such sequencing. Less frequent were recur- rent behavior sequences across subjects within a session (inter -individual), as is shown in the second column of Table 10. Early in training, when much competing and accessory behavior was occurring in similar cate- gories and locus for all the subjects, it was difficult to discern such

WS*'^ f t ‘•O'.:

^ M-* .‘ “'■ -■‘' ' <

'i~

' '•-'3 i"' i®''

..j

j I

>1 ^-. )An o'* T;.t ■• •• : b^W'.:’ r. i:*3-.,C'irl#

-, >tr

r. .iftiJiK n.5f .•>-it:/m’'l‘«dfvir 5, ^

-

M^iJ

I.M-' r,iL# t i^.- riA. 7$ -

' -/ 0 r'fi. •'<'• -■ >a vIofil^^2]bl\nl fcicin . 'd'';_

■;.‘1 1 . U) f 44^i. 0 ‘jt i 'J fc:r.' o

' . '■'*’T~' •^'"^^Jhil-i-^; I* ■''

'I 1 J 0 li-v A , ' ':;

4«,

<^i »

«t..i.n i'

4j.'» tJ «fi6f-T? SA- ^ije 'filO'lu'

•^: '^ •■■ '“ ■■■■ 3lj

■■ '. 1 ■• :• ,■ ■";•?-■■" ■••• t4'i; H-T^itM %:

A ., .\ . »»S-=' .'i'Av.'yplfce

k‘

5»! .,=

j;

; , ' ' ■'■■■" " '. ^' r .

'' " \‘i^ ^ .. ^ -- ■'-

A

£. -'Vir': ' iv-

- - "

>«>e '• 'i^r-o.'' •.‘s»>*'<u 'HrA( ^joii* a.i.i*,«u*i m

■■.. .

^ '30 . . V; .

: ■• . ni ■> S.! J'r../- .»ri.

' '.,■ ■■ ^ . O;’ '■ ■! "^' '^- '.'

- .‘-niih .:,i ai!.:./Vwb,, £irw il -ilA -. "Itoji

I. *

'»•

a

71

Table 10

Proportion of CB and AB Trials Showing Intra- and Inter - Individual Behavior Sequences in Experiment I

Subjects

Intra - Individual

Inte r - Individual

Gp H

101

0

0

109

0

. 04

105

. 18

. 03

113

. 21

. 03

108

.29

0

no

. 11

0

X

. 13

. 02

Gp T

102

0

. 19

112

.54

. 08

103

.11

. 18

107

. 70

. 16

106

.46

. 35

111

0

. 07

X

. 30

. 17

72

sequences since the "signal -to -noise" ratio was so small. After a few trials, when the overall level of competing and accessory behavior had decreased, and they occurred mainly in and involved few categories, the few instances of inter -individual sequencing were more easily identi- fied. These sequences also primarily comprised bouts of sniffing, most often sniffing a circumscribed part of the floor, suggesting the tracing of other subjects by odor. This result is surprising since the floor of the alleyway was always thoroughly scrubbed with Lysol disinfectant and dried after every run, and subjects were never seen to engage in any behavior which would obviously correspond to leaving a pheromonal cue. Other competing and accessory behavior often occurred on trials showing inter - individual sequencing.

Neither shifts in reward magnitude nor the introduction of the insect- icide stimulus were associated with an inordinate amount of sequencing of either type. The occasional periods of atypically long run times for sev- eral subjects, seen for example around trials 11-15 and 21-25 (Figs. 2 and 3), also did not involve disproportionate levels of intra- or inter - individual sequencing. There were wide individual differences within each group in the frequency of such sequences, especially intra-individual.

Both kinds of sequencing were much more frequent overall for Gp T than

Gp H.

•7 *

T

D..

-•

terf

V«^- : 'T* i (•-■• *‘ .'V - t ^

# r..-.' * ^ , .#« , .•: ,S(»'

- ^ ;■ ■■ 'i ' '

. •*'• ►.!».' l>..- ii^v* ^' !■■

*•: -I*- « ..Ti > .■■- ■*.f

6ti J

.'Cr ^ ^ .

^ "Vlil sif ifj , 'I

» » -.

^ »

««

I » I

jft ■••"•i'a ■IS”. - '

. I t

, V -' . * f ' ^

. - : ■_ f 1

' - (' . ■•* ' 1

I

%r'^v ''‘I* -ion

5 " (

. * * iV.I I

, ^ 'iV:* OtV'?

^ , p ilUv :>i:f

) f'\‘ I .

•M : ->(M« •■.fi- lit’ T('iu' ll«i

. t»! l- ii ^k%7 l^<aM,»;’i:4i rJf;;^*r»:jcav‘«6t

, . '. ■,- ' > '■,' ■■■

I r

, »

'^■<v-^l ■■>> .i«r''-4'’j «r .*j -fv . .vjji' isi*j*a \46. ittm

>.M,

* i;i<i

1 .f ir'

1 •, Tf^i:r i^’ f&JI^

ri-u.-i i rfiJi' '. J8|»^ API^^ _ ,

■’ ■'-■ ' 'I'-is! r^y

c‘.. '-t

ijt.

•»*W

"<h*-

■1 -“t ' '

73

Discussion

The present results demonstrate that the decrease in overall run time in the alleyway in the course of acquisition for individual rats reflects a large-scale, orderly decrease in the amount of competing and accessory behavior, but that the vigor of running also increases, within a much nar- rower range. Thus, the results of previous studies of competing behavior in alleyway acquisition {Cicala, 1961; Kello, unpublished experiment;

King, 1959; Marx & Brownstein, 1963; Pereboom & Crawford, 1958;

Smoot, 1964) are supported for single subjects, with rigorous measures of competing behavior and the running response, and these results are ex- tended to include thirsty rats running to water. Hence, both the Hullian S-R view and the response -competition view of learning in the alleyway are shown to be inaccurate. However, both positions coxild easily be modified to account for the observed structure of acquisition in the alleyway. The S-R view would have to acknowledge the response -competition claim that all learning is selective learning, that even the "ideally simple" alleyway is necessarily a multiple -response, trial -and-error situation. Thus, fol- lowing the S-R account of selective learning, with practice an instrumental response does increase slightly in vigor (provided that response vigor is correlated with reinforcement; cf. , Logan, 1959, I960), but more impor- tantly, it competes more and more successfully with alternative responses, i.e., occurs more often (Hull, 1930; Kimble, 1961, p. 408; Siegel, 1945; Spence, 1956, 1958, I960). The response -competition view would have to

■. ' 'v

i»'tttlf'^'i ' ..ir.

'^"y' :fT|

4 '■' 4Si *4

■* ■?.*.« , * i «..■■. .r<IR(v* « 1'^ ' '• , .V i , -: - *, ! ■<>: i '/ : >W I ' i

Swill*

ll.s .'i

. : Ur. ■-! '"N;

1^

- . 1 ' -, 7 ; '' , t ,nt>^ir7i Jl

A !-,vrrt't:44^-^i.,r' i

' ' _ V^v

j .11 ('f f*.'- ■■■i/' I**'*'" ■•'’ '•'^ -‘‘ * ■fetf t>^' '5v||M|J1"'^~~‘' f

' '/I

i .

k\ ..-i ^ ^

;. K Y^-

■!■ >V- ... 4/<*'A> : n.».»^» *••

•• - . t 1 1 !*rjy _;: .tt :'cr-

I. •<

“'' ■■■■ «•- ..■’ '■' ' XrSS*®

"' .. .„ la 0

>\i iiiv y

^S.'

I . / ; t"-** >"-Tr.‘: - '-* w:

I •I ^ . it 0 1 j ... 's t fi t>»

- j V.' O'®!

'■ V - .' " 'V*- ' / M '^’

U' U* ♦'-!

r ; . t

f i

,V ■' ' * ■•'.•? <*.f ' f !».’<, Mivff

- ^ J

»V. 'i»Uipi»..*.-.» j;ii.’w 67 /.JW

i^iill .J(

.. .. !'<•*-■ Iitr.v* •#'.*.*•■ )7.-'i,» * .yai; o> 4jfc.a)>q«^' i'' t’lf"'

' '"■ a

L

i.

74

acknowledge that in the absence of overt competing behavior the vigor of most instrumental responses can vary at least within a narrow range, and that such responses become effectively all-or-none responses only when practiced extensively (cf . , Bolles, 1958b), as in free-operant situations.

It is seen here that the vigor of noninstrumental behavior also varies, from full competing forms to weaker accessory forms.

The present results show that, as with acquisition, large changes in overall run time induced by other variables during rewarded training (dif- ferences in amount of reward in acquisition, shifts in amount of reward, introduction of an extra stimulus, and proximity to reward) also reflect changes in the amount of competing and accessory behavior, but that smaller changes in the rate of running per se also occur. This was illus- trated in some detail for individual rats, both hungry and thirsty. It was found that amount of reward affects the rate of acquisition much more than the final level of performance at asymptote (Hammer, 1971; Pereboom & Crawford, 1958), though there were wide individual differences in the rate and orderliness of acquisition. The present results do not support Ham- mer's (1971) conclusion that, when competing behavior has dropped out, shifts in reward magnitude have little effect. The apparent magnitude of upshift effects was indeed small under these circumstances as Hammer showed, almost certainly reflecting a "ceiling effect" (Bower, 1961). But downshifts in amount of reward here at middle and late stages of training when runs were mainly stable DR's continued to have large decremental

I** *-1'

%

M* 1

l

4

...s

”" *M -a. ''4 i

, ' ■/.■ -■< A ii!

I r;*

m i . :.

- 1

'. •- , »!>- *IwS -»i, '

't.

»Wit? *r «0

I U^ iio

^9' *' '* ?3^

' ’'"C^Sr . ica ^ ' ' »

V. -iC -ir , ,,, -j,

1. -

0.0^1

rlfcefl'^ .''^‘ i«’ ■> ‘’Vif •' r / j, .,i fH . ,j,]^r ...^%f vtf ia^yi^W «4:t?##.;,

*lii^4.'**: 16^' #'jB;t3i9«5'^--itt«'t^ VifT \rrrcj^j|^u>p^1fb’’^^r'itU4:v^

fW »• ib tvafi - ' gi^.

-■»!

V "'fir'

-pj

-I * '.4i ^ i4»

Sa

3

K#*

, . : ' ':t^3sum^

Lit*'' - - -' :»'-/rVd‘'OlJf, i^<lid;.^t>^ ^'^<2 -,;j,|ist3r' P.-ttf J "''3

'■.-m^:'' ' -r---

,.« i,t.<i-'i l t » » ■';4i^ii ft ‘'■r '-

r.i.

75

effects on running for subjects who had not previously experienced the small reward magnitude. Hammer's data also show a downshift effect, though smaller than observed here.

The temporary disruptive effect of the insecticide extra stimulus observed here for most subjects of both groups seems comparable to "external inhibition" as reported by Pavlov (1927). Similar disruptions of an ongoing instrumental response have often been reported in the alley- way (Courtney, Reid, &; Wasden, 1968; Gagne, 1941; Kello, 1972; McNamara & Wike, 1958; Winnick & Hunt, 1951) and in other situations as well (Bindra, 1961; Bruce, 1935; Hoffman & Overman, 1971; Smith, 1971; Wendt, 1936). The effects of the extra stimulus in the present experiment looked generally like the effects of a downshift in reward mag- nitude. In both cases the structure of behavior on the long trials imme- diately following these events closely resembled that on the long trials earlier in acquisition, a similarity suggested before in the maze -learning literature (Bruce, 1935). With prolonged exposure to the extra stimulus, as well as to reduced reward magnitude, run times decreased, and the underlying behavioral changes were similar to those seen earlier in acquisition.

The goal gradient in overall run time was not an "artifact" of com- peting behavior (Marx & Brownstein, 1963). The pattern was also shown by all of the hungry subjects and most of the thirsty subjects on the major- ity of their DR trials, on which no competing or accessory behavior

4U«

v' *.Al ;, 'fXt^ ■■■•.;. ,. ••' . . ' ’,4!^'..'^^.-

•'■ ';"•'! "’ ' , /' :

tllkvist ni j|^.* ' »i/ •♦ 1’*' ’wi'3.

'* .i«: I •< nihf.*

'"<l}' Itf ’Wfiort^ad hiatSaraB^IK

;": \ '^■'>¥'' ■»: 4--/'

).¥l »- t> it3^'tk&

<i >Virlia ^ f>jrv-NtigS-Jmltf A

a., \ , ^

i“.f !t»»hf-¥ »u-'M '-v<,,,,j ,,i iLi-oira

’■• f

t: w'siru -XfJ^ i '. ' '''; ' .‘’I

y<f .

*^r ‘t* ^ .^^^JLiwiri .1 1 V J

, ' , ' a "'"■

‘■' rt. »M 'L-^’ f.

't?l!!lStiwi>.a *ti Atfi'ISrc

III'' W-‘ •'^

:>ViUUW*r-'" fxiVioM.': ui’

I

;*»»'■ ;.- v , ' '

.;d

76

occurred. The slope of the gradient was, however, enhanced by the occurrence of competing and accessory behaviors, as King (1959) found, since such behaviors occurred most frequently in the first alleyway seg- ment (cf. , Marx &: Brownstein, 1963).

The possibility that the observed variation in the rate of running in acquisition and with the other manipulations studied here might be due to the occurrence of overt competing behavior falling outside the usual defi- nition cannot be strongly argued. First, subcriterial noninstrumental behaviors such as head swings occurred on less than 2% of all rated DR trials, and not preferentially on the slowest. Second, Premack (1965) has suggested, without elaboration, that rats may have several constant '*^intra -burst" rates of running corresponding to different gaits. If this were indeed the case, and if it could be argued that slower gaits can be considered competing behaviors, then a strict response -competition view of learning and performance in the alleyway could still be maintained. Setting aside the problems raised by extending the definition of competing behavior to include slower gaits, this position can be rejected on the basis of the present finding that run times on DR trials showing different gaits overlapped extensively.

The thirsty, water -rewarded rats showed much more variability in run time and more competing behavior throughout the experiment than the hungry, food -rewarded rats. Also, with the exception that rat 108 of Gp H showed much more accessory behavior than any thirsty subject (see

«iit '^»*l 4Mb 7. j.

*»■

». r I

i »0 ’',-.'>*v'*fHt''^|

R'

- - s?

ft

t . jj* flt,a3 y.

..vf' ^

!

I :- o/J J

. Ill

Mi t ^lii

' ■' ‘J

r « t ' V , . ;, jT ! I J ; /-v ;'|t«ul '6 1 1 1 rt;M m ■. > ar =rx S>xfS

>S'’ : ;,<■: ■;', ~ A..

M-i> .-ss-.r

( I H I

« i

1 . - ' - ^ c>. . -6 y (ajj CJ^'-'Yi(§ CBa t'JUjj-A 3t. «

V" - "' :: ''■■$®'^” :^...

. -’®

■iC

1 n

1'

i> I

1^ -, , 1®!®“''^ "

,.s }t^-, 4;r. .li o44itA lfsftif;4-

5U : ~> - ?#■

bTri<iJS^,jf

’jh •• t'

■A;' ** is.,- ^

iStf*- ■^r

^). . ,^-

li^ ,^ i r- ry'^'rf)S4?' f 1 i^^Q; a> '

'U'i . ■■ .:Xoo run.

-•r .' ' ,V"1 'm ' '?!

¥-t ■^'W-m

■" m" ■"

3

■'fid

•■', "' 'i :i

jmtS r^cri:; ■'••;4h

'(K* '51,

,,.ii W

'3||Wi*rtiN H cfD

77

Table 9), Gp T generally showed more accessory behavior than Gp Overall, the behavior of the thirsty subjects appeared to be less affected by reinforcement than did the behavior of the hungry subjects. Thus, acquisition was slower for Gp T, and behavior was more variable in the presence of the extra stim\ilus (cf . , Bruce, 1935) but usually less affected by upshifts or downshifts in amount of reward. It was also observed that the thirsty subjects showed a weaker goal gradient both overall and on DR trials alone, and that they galloped less and showed recurrent sequences of competing and accessory behavior more often than did the hungry sub- jects.

The finding that the performance of thirsty, water -rewarded rats is more variable than the performance of hungry, food-rewarded ones in the alleyway is directly counter to the implication of the results of T-maze and general activity studies comparing hungry and thirsty rats, and at least one alleyway study (Bolles, 1958a; Campbell & Cicala, 1961; Hall, 1955; Kintsch, 1962; Petrinovich & Bolles, 1954). It is also counter to the generalization in the older literature (e.g., Munn, 1950), based on Warden’s (1931) studies vdth the Columbia obstruction apparatus, that 24- hr. thirst is "more m.otivating" than 24-hr. hunger. Rather, the present results closely parallel those reported by Bruce (1935) with hungry and thirsty rats in a multiple -iinit maze. Bruce found that 24-hr. thirsty rats ran more slowly, made more errors, and showed more trial -to -trial variability in both performance measures than 24-hr. hungry rats.

.ill qO Mftft

* \*

■„«' :■

3Ton: .

*'' v. ..

'■ -■* " ,^.. ^

. <►. » »

,"/^'

*A ..,. MT« 4J a. '•<!(>

I -. , »'lJ*’*i:l i '

•% ;-. r/.'. Air l9hA 1

•» s

,f t

-i-'f <f .i.'it. t 'e'’

» ,*:l . ' . «■ , < lx ;. -‘

' or •,v.. ^

t yij|pi}isii Ivy o-'»iiii:ir*fa*T«i‘4 aVil^#44/^ ''':

^': f

“!

MjJ lA adt r.'i ^

^4c! :'Jr'

l*«

ri>. .-J ?r- t?!li. yifsKUa

V ^ ^ . 'M'1 .5^..,

-t -^*.^.v,, v _ <. J ( r.

ftw t r- ■» «" ' ^ ! '

9f i i-

iff » It 1 ^ -.

**i ' ;.'vij»:jif iA -fS^

^ V

'■ . "■ •■ ■■" 'P' ■?;

'Ul ' I } d”;..- .'^'i 5 li- wsMcf . 1^1 ansa^t: _ai£J .I

*

ik

'( --; .1“ a.-Hj !’^i }o'J> Bi'.li

"i, '^'^'■li’ ■"" ■■'

‘t ^ : y.»i"Kj:i « .'Tx-fii '-' 3;iTri} " ^irf j^..;

.' ' ' ' .X, 'i '*>5^

tU bffv/v>tk ■^i;^',fe't*v« tjs fliiaf.'

‘."rtl- ’’i rtf 'i ifT-'f j,'i.*x.,i>fr> iijawff.j^jjj|p|(|j|^»'^

78

The differences found here between hungry and thirsty rats might simply reflect quantitative differences in drive. Indeed, while the sub- jects of Gp H lost to 80-90% ad lib body weight in the course of the experi- ment, Gp T showed only a temporary loss followed by a gain back to approximately 100% ad lib weight, and in some cases more. Such changes are common for hungry and thirsty rats at this level of deprivation (Bolles & Petrinovich, 1956). To determine whether the observed differences between the hungry and thirsty rats reflect quantitative or qualitative dif- ferences in drive, it would be necessary to vary the deprivation level of subjects in one motivational condition to see if a point can be reached at which their performance corresponds to that of subjects in the other moti- vational condition at some fixed deprivation level (Shettleworth, 1972); this is a variant of Bitterman's (I960) "control by systematic variation."

It is also possible that the observed differences in performance might reflect, to some extent, differences in amount of reward. It might be that at the drive levels used, say, five times as much water as was given would have resulted in performance for Gp T that was comparable to that for Gp H. In the absence of parametric studies of this type, the reasons for the difference in variability between the two groups cannot be deter- mined.

However, at least one result suggests that quantitative differences in drive or reward magnitude may not be sufficient to account for the behavioral differences between the two groups. The thirsty subjects

a

^' ' iiL.^ ® J*' 1

4f»%. f.»! •l/t-r* -VI.0.1 .VMO iJJ *

.. , .., - - ,

V

,•■< )“ w»ii»3'; /t,.- a '<>l^ ■•<'■' J ayl'io

'M-. ‘nea ^

V ^>e^

' V j ti/ -SfC 14'fr'l ^ ^ ^.n i Jtj-' Tt'dt ' ^i*.-:' ^^'fcjF'te

-. ' . .-it . ■ttiilw' (W^fTt'iy’ jo ...T ^

,7^

■I t •_ r4»4Ci<*7

at

T Yk^dt!'jO‘‘rit '-:t i.lim/. if

I ' . . :

'■ ur'^r:.

1-'.:|.1.... I •’■•.' a.’’lMs^''V -:u-3

t fj'^- , nq<dS> f\ vf>a‘,^i< t)is l&a0^.JS^^-‘ “I

•J^f I"'

> A *# -

|., .,)!•». I .••.'• ''i.lPi, i « >. "if

•'•* . .11.- bt»-. *f%!*U^'.

* .. ’’r •■'h ru a.f|3ix!' t*^i,i.('

T- •>. ■:- '-.-fl* _-,_

'• ' f,'.\' I if-ro

i*> ’'■ 1 !•

tiu ?y'/.i ,viv

■■■ .

. I Zirv . r.i , I [HllJUW' " '

.t -t

v/i -• .4i *'-<‘-»7sr.*-i-;f ’MiiMy \&y '^oi-

M ;r^- jllm cJT -j-tn r/^xy!9|'''^

; (_-• -, -. .

jJ I

1 -T 1 •'

-• :i #. 7i y X iV 1 i>jLf » ' f mriicb ai

1 UA'^'lH

i-f:

79

showed a small average goal gradient, and showed this pattern on a rela- tively small proportion of their trials; some individuals of Gp T showed a negative goal gradient on the average (cf . , Bruce, 1937). For hungry rats it is known that the average goal gradient is sharper with small amounts of reward (Crespi, 1942; Hull, 1934) and lower levels of depriva- tion (Hull, 1934). While decreasing the amount of reward or level of deprivation for hungry rats might result in as slow a rate of acquisition, as much competing and accessory behavior, and as much trial -to -trial variability as seen here for the thirsty rats, these operations should fur- ther enhance the goal gradient rather than weakening it. Thus the differ- ence in the goal gradient for hungry and thirsty rats, as shown here, may in fact reflect a qualitative difference as a function of motivational condi- tion, though further study is required to bear this out.

Reasons for the failure to find qualitative differences, or indeed any large quantitative differences, in the categories of behavior for the hungry and thirsty rats are not clear. It may be that over a wide range of condi- tions the same "appetitive behaviors" (Craig, 1918) for rats are asso- ciated with both hunger and thirst (see Hull, 1943), and that the alleyway does not provide stimulus conditions appropriate to separate hunger- and thirst-related behaviors. There has also been a failure to find qualitative differences in the "free behavior" of hungry and thirsty rats in simple open-field situations (Kello, unpublished observations; Prescott, 1970). Perhaps enriching either situation and providing opportunities for a wider

» ' ^ ... ■!»

-.'T . Cig>^jro?yb iKljl

#J #

i»ii« fiit

m # IC

» -

A4 >4.f^ wkrtMB t.'>', :' ,*■!'■

Ui'’ r. '^1

* jgt ‘,ij "

-. r«\ ll/rti-: » ’S'! 'V

r .t^i^'.' ,. -i •’•ro*?.4 A'V>fi itoas

■'■ . ," 4^" '^{':

.■■ - ■tc,'-‘i’ vTfiru.ii rcl'J . v"^ •? ^^0

■>.M '.(M

' ' ' ' ' \ ' ■'’'■-'■lijg, Ifi*;.

'■"sfCi-':. f 'J

I : V ><' ^ V i ’iol'

.,,-j;: e, . ,>t: ' t S ;5t ■' ' i nxiikii^ _ ''

I >1

(i*

'" '■' ' .. ' .«. : '

-:j -»■■■)

m.

- , ■• V: '

1 - ■.,.f<':i;o «.*'

i'v ''‘- r>

■s0-^ m»^jiia^s biiih’

\' :i. ' '^■•'' '

'"- ^ . 1. ^

VI.' 'i-' -Ur ®'' f

rintv-Tp.

80

range of behavior (e. g. , adding areas for digging, side alleys, or objects to manipulate) would reveal differences as a function of motivational state. Or, it might be that, in the unfamiliar alleyway situation, exploratory or curiosity behaviors predominate and interfere with the expression of hunger- or thirst-related behaviors. Although extraneous novel stimuli will engender competing and accessory behavior in the alleyway, strong arguments can be made against the common view that such behaviors are simply a response to unfamiliarity (e. g. , Bindra, 1961; Estes, 1958;

Hinde, 1970; Pereboom, 1957). For example, even when maximal these behaviors do not appear as a thorough exploration of the environment.

Many parts of the apparatus are never "explored" by a given subject throughout training, while other parts, such as the floor in Aj^, are examined repeatedly. Also, competing and accessory behaviors occur as described even when subjects have been given an extensive period of pre- experimental exploration of the alleyway, rewarded or unrewarded (Bindra, 1963; Hammer, 1971; Kello, unpublished experiment; Pereboom & Crawford, 1958), and they may continue to occur for many hundreds of training trials (Cotton, 1953). Moreover, it is not clear in what sense the same environment should be more "novel" for subjects who are under weaker drive conditions or who are receiving smaller reward. It is also not clear why a novel decrease in reward magnitude should engender com- peting behavior while a novel increase does not.

It is not necessary to assume that competing and accessory behaviors

'■% . Ji

4. « i »f#

■•■ " V I vf f '►v

:Si;

.1 . 1

f ,' A/t*' '<y.*5’‘ f<^li j ')*> ■>

, ■■' t j. .fy-.* 'i-.

.| ■•.. f'<5 <*

.. ..

.i.lii > ,"*Ti , i'

' ..•■ '■' ' . .'j *%),

' jagp^tg ..

7Tr»;9H40 f

'■'■ SbT^ •■'

-t'v Uf .3*1

fc**U4, -, ■■’ A.- ^1' 4«d

JO ■-'( I I •- ,'

w.' ^

|?'i :*'* '

c . f\ fi» aSiW;* V '*•*

I'i.

■'

*•

*v» +.

i I ’► .M.

'h/, lU ,;ial

L "ifrP

■*^ .. >mffj ?;k.L w-A >

I vr4'%0 4'’ -‘S'-

iC-’

o'ikipS%v ir*m »t‘( ivoit.'

-■■'^i..' ■■' ' ■■' •?! '* ■'._

' l'‘ : :^0¥}4z>* *.'■* - j^‘''<

'J^

hr ,i: -Ky. ^ - ., ' w *

.4 41

'irriiltiiiri' >jV: 4*

-

.7f‘'„

81

are exploratory behaviors elicited by novelty, or that they are random, unstructured, spontaneous activities. The view of the alleyway as a selective learning situation, supported here, suggests another account of these behaviors. Thus, running and the other behaviors may be seen as reflecting an organized response hierarchy, an initial range of non- random behavioral variation in the alleyway (though it is still not clear whyliunger and thirst did not have differential effects on the range or relative frequency of behaviors in the hierarchy). During rewarded train- ing the "other" behaviors which compete with the rewarded running response are selected out in an orderly way. Their occurrence becomes less frequent, and when they do occur they take up less time and occur in fewer bouts, involving fewer categories. Further, full competing behav- ior drops out faster than weaker accessory forms. Also, after a few trials these behaviors, especially full -competing forms, generally occur within a trial only where the selection for running is weakest, viz. , in the early parts of the alleyway, farthest from reward. ^

Note that this observation suggests similarities between alleyway competing behavior and "interim activities" (Staddon Sz Simmelhag, 1971) and "adjunctive behaviors" (Falk, 1970) seen in some free -operant situa- tions. These behaviors are all apparently irrelevant noninstrumental activities which occur primarily where reinforcement probability is lowest, viz., spatially or temporally farthest from reward. That these behaviors are not all identical, however, is suggested by the observation that alley- way competing behavior is most salient early and decreases with training, while interim and adjunctive behaviors develop and become more stereo- typed with training.

u

nri'

^ *^« ^

SS' 38 Jgg ^ '^/.'.r-.v..

'. -s' ' "

ti,. <m»i^>. -'•ir

'li i J| ^')^>'%^Si^p||?a 'di't » (ti ,,%

'n£IU t. »«J>I?«J f>*«*.* _ K’ -

:'" .^PS '4 A:'i

I -..I v-'V*^ ••■'■’ -O Igito'. :ili-a '.Wl'toir'.^^*? .a '

t iv

f . '^ V. . ,|

(v4a ■ia’inU ^ .a. ,, f

•■i*-?* jl' c^

»

•'m .■

i^#ltrW\/'J fe-

i' ,. .— » gfci - i ■#'

I i?. - ^ - -^ '■ I

i .V- .'■,' ■' .,'■': ,'' "

«:)| t^'.'it^' S ■' i . I ,■ ' ■*4^l( 'y4i4i.' Ki5?^3<-'' ^ ^

-'•^J.JW ... _

-^.■s>*-v^ .'! -*!*&.] ••■ ’-Ml-

.. .. , . 1 „._. . ^

*«A# ( *.l|K r* *.*A' < I ) ^^|p| t, ‘<.'044..

. H^«''iiUf ^;'.^||^ Jiirtr*?.!!’ ,#fiil''-isiplJ5^|i^

■■' ''*' ^'\v . u ■4j5' fY .i.cJ^iftltFirW4’^ si6' -^

i

t

itt*'

v«.' '. . if j»vux4jattiuTi.;5* >

'■^Qi

- ■^.~ , ~ -y a- --.fc— -A-!;.- J ■■.•!?•• ■■•jrf-'^ir-- -• . .^;..I^-.

' -J ..| U-ffiuUt *

>-?f>i.T''r' •**' T|l<f ■»» .1i.t»..i)'^is >'^^:.

•^■•'r^t.'rt:

W'

jp:_ i6-t4 ^ v':^' ita. . f" iS>«i*Hi T .*'i! aoif' '

.;v ^iM^ ®<i|H .'« : .

,‘vmH ^'ii^' - y'y

82

It may be assumed that this selection process is situation -dependent. Thus a change in environmental conditions, not involving reward itself, engenders a temporary return in competing and accessory behavior of the same form as earlier in acquisition, with the same range and approxi- mately the same rank-order of categories of competing and accessory behavior. An abrupt decrease in amount of reward involves directly weakening the selection for running. Therefore, downshifts have effects similar to the introduction of extra stimuli but, at least for hungry, food- rewarded subjects, even greater. In both cases, under the continuing selective effect of reinforcement, competing and accessory behaviors drop out as in acquisition. An abrupt increase in amount of reward increases the strength of selection for running, so that no competing or accessory behavior occurs. Up to this point, this account may be seen as an elabora- tion of a consistent S-R reinforcement view of selective learning, or as a variety of the response -competition view which attributes the dropping out of competing behavior specifically to the selective effect of reinforce- ment (cf. , Staddon, 1972; Staddon & Simmelhag, 1971). The mechanism underlying the consistent changes in the vigor of running observed here, independent of any overt competing behavior, is not clear. These changes may reflect a direct effect of reinforcement on the strength of the selected response, or extinction "below zero" of (covert) competing behavior ten- dencies, or both. Observed quantitative variations in noninstrumental behaviors, from accessory to competing forms, pose a similar problem.

{.4t ^ ' *'

83

The occasional occurrence of repetitive sequences of competing and accessory behavior within- and between-subjects, and of periods of atypically long run times for several subjects, largely independent of such sequencing, have been observed before in the alleyway, but they have received little attention and are not well understood (Kello, unpublished experiment; Ludvigson Sz Sytsma, 1967; Pereboom & Crawford, 1958).

In addition to the low overall frequency of such patterns, there is further evidence that the structure of behavior observed here was not dependent upon inter -subject interactions. Pilot experiments have shown that behav- ior in acquisition and in the presence of extra stimuli looks the same as shown here even when single subjects are run by themselves to the com- pletion of training (Kello, unpublished experiment; see also Pereboom &

Crawford, 1958).

EXPERIMENT n

A further implication of the view of the alleyway as a multiple - response, selective -learning situation was examined in this experiment. Since the removal of. reward in extinction would eliminate selection for running, competing and accessory behavior of the same form as in the earliest stages of acquisition should return throughout the alleyway. And, since the rate of running varies little, at least in rewarded training, this return in competing and accessory behavior should largely account for the great increase in overall run time seen in extinction. Although there has been no detailed single -subject analysis of alleyway behavior in extinction, theoretical accounts of extinction in terms of "interfering response" tendencies are common, not only among response -competition theorists, but paradoxically, among some Hullian S-R theorists as well (Amsel, 1958; Estes, 1959; Guthrie, 1959; Kimble, 1961; Spence, I960; Wendt, 1936). Since S-R theorists accord no role to interfering responses during continuously rewarded acquisition, they generally account for the occurrence of these behaviors in extinction by assuming that they are elicited by nonreward (e.g., "emotional" or "frustration" behaviors, Amsel, 1958; see Kimble, 1961; Spence, I960). On the empirical side.

84

. ■• t' I* r . 'Vrtr *■ *’ ^ 4'^ '

. * V

(. ij

! J .. *

I I

■‘>:M::!sir7U'-» ''In I.'"' ta i.9 {^5i,

" iir VW?<- '’’ ' Jtt . - &

-uo/t,y .,. ;...^^

I - ' r.

’*1^4

% * . >¥■' ;. ^

■vj

- iV':->V

85

several studies have reported the general finding that competing behavior is in fact quite prominent in extinction and may thus account for part of the increase in overall run time, though it is not clear how much (e. , Bindra, 1963; Marx & Brownstein, 1963; McCoy & Marx, 1965). Further, it is sometimes reported that specific categories of behavior seen earlier in acquisition are particularly likely in extinction, though it is not clear whether the range of behavior is extended in extinction (Hull, 1934; Kello, unpublished observations; Mackintosh, 1955; McNamara 8z Wike, 1958; Miller & Miles, 1936; Miller &; Stevenson, 1936; Pryor, Haag, & O'Reilly, 1969; Ross, 1964). The present experiment attempted to provide the cur- rently lacking descriptive analysis of the behavior of individual rats run- ning to nonreward in extinction following continuously rewarded acquisi- tion, and to thereby determine the form of competing behavior, and its contribution to the increase in overall run time, in extinction.

A1 so, a prediction not critical to the view of the alleyway as a selec- tive learning situation was tested here, in two ways. It might be assumed that the more competing behavior which occurs on rewarded trials, the more that competing behavior tendencies are selected against, and thus the greater the relative strength of the tendency to run once running pre- dominates (Staddon, 1972). The stronger the tendency to run, the more nonrewarded trials it should take in extinction before competing behavior returns. Thus, it may be predicted that any manipulation which encour- ages the temporary expression of competing behavior on rewarded trials

U i

. M

', .M f#,3lt.2 •» -, .■, f .sl/'K-tlJ ,

' IH iS

,j , ,:> -1 wvi*'

©

f,

SVVi.

cn

-. » '■■

i

I.. . « .-rt •■: •? ,'• ••.jfr ■■} S\ ‘1 '■ ' .

." ' . . . / 'I li^ iwiMA

W , 1 L , ♦) '.!

«\f. ’*»'

- ‘.'3

-"Tiy- ™'"’ . ' T

i 'A

^(J

... r.,j "i''i ,'■ ■.** au r

‘i >.-^'

v; ^^•■'

V ' .-V ■♦u -'r '

..*>s.

/?»•!

M3 W- ! .y

Ar.

'*'tl

„•■<■ v"'

■■ft >l;t -i tiA *ltS*

•..' »ir

■■«’ "'liH

j

•> (\. •' ij/f .i/iit, ’I*. (I

■■r^i

n !>.- -. f-^ jj-:. « i't*-o''^i.

[■ X

site:-,. -i^ T - ;,«• ■("

»'«i<

fr,V"j ■■

86

should increase resistance to extinction (see Amsel, 1972, for a similar prediction, and Bindra, 1961, for the opposite prediction). Consistent with this prediction, McNamara and Wike (1958) found that group- averaged resistance to extinction was increased by stimulus changes in acquisition unrelated to reward, though they did not present data showing to what extent these stimulus changes increased the amount of competing behavior in acquisition. It is also suggestive that subjects trained with small reward, and therefore presumably showing more competing behav- ior in acquisition, sometimes show greater resistance to extinction than subjects trained with large reward (Robbins, 1971). To test the prediction that the more competing behavior temporarily induced in acquisition, the greater the resistance to extinction, the present experiment compared the acquisition and extinction behavior of continuously rewarded rats with and without extra stimuli in acquisition.

One manipulation which reliably produces greater resistance to extinction is partial reinforcement (PRF) in acquisition. Does partial reinforcement yield more competing behavior in acquisition, before stable DR trials predominate, than does continuous reinforcement (CRF)? This is a fairly common prediction (e.g., Amsel, 1958, 1972; Estes, 1959; Weinstock, 1954). Several studies, inspired by the frustration theory (Amsel, 1958) or "habituation" (Weinstock, 1954) accounts of the partial reinforcement effect on extinction, in terms of interfering response ten- dencies during PRF acquisition, have compared the frequency of one or

0- >,

' ' '4; ,,■'' ■’^'’

P?' , ,ti!^ .1/ ,'■

* ". 1 ."'-■ asMiiH V** f i- '•’' II

.V'. ^ .ixi ^;.TP't,!^^'jn<* '< ' m ^

.»;,•' ‘i 1 (V 1/^ ft .'I » ): < ^-Ja\

t ' ■: (V‘ ? , ,0 •*'- : -j (• -, " ttj' ) ’^n

i ' ''

ni

'^4 4S

^... 'i ^'* '.

87

two competing behaviors for CRF and PRF groups, but the results have been inconclusive (Robbins, 19 71). Often both groups show the same amount overall (e.g., McCoy & Marx, 1965), but sometimes the PRF group shows slightly more competing behavior when its run time is longer (early acquisition) and less when its run time is shorter (late acquisition) than the CRF group (Goodrich, 1959; Jones & Bridges, 1966; McCoy &; Marx, 1965). The possibility remains that the partial reinforcement effect may depend, not on specific experience with nonreward in acquisi- tion, but only on the fact that such experience will strongly encourage the behavioral expression of competing behavior, thereby ultimately strength- ening the tendency to run relative to the tendencies to engage in competing behaviors, and resulting in greater persistence of running when rev/ard is completely removed. The present experiment tested this possibility by comparing the behavior of continuously and partially reinforced rats in acquisition and extinction, using strict measures of competing behavior and the instrumental response, as in Experiment I.

Method

Subjects and apparatus

The subjects were 10 male Long-Evans hooded rats approximately 120 days old at the first experimental session. The alleyway and record- ing equipment were the same as in Experiment I.

‘■1l#’.Vl^-’n *

. i '[,,4 t;f'

■' 't-^-'w'ii^® . ■ti' ,*' - ..-

0 "r

’• *• 'm

.czS" t^imS*-" ‘s "T ^..

:-T«;

•«s-^ ■' '• fi, *'?atif^" ' '.,^-

'- .1 J j"-?rf5!:r ■■•" ;**i-?^--.. . ■!

f-v ^T; , . ,," '

. ■*■- •ur~ ,: ,

'\ itf\&

■’■. '

. I , C' i ‘v.t^ ',.<!:/ :j*iu ^

88

Pretraining

Upon arrival in the laboratory the subjects were caged singly in 24-hr. light and given food and water ad lib. They were handled at least 2-3 min. on each of the first 7 days and were handled and gentled inter- mittently across the remaining 36 days of pretraining. On pretraining day 20 all subjects were placed on a 1-hr. per day restricted feeding schedule, with water still available ad lib, on which they were maintained throughout the experiment. On days 35-43 the subjects were brought to the experimental room separately in three groups, at the approximate time they would be run during the experiment. Each group was exposed to the experimental room, set up for a session, and to typical equipment noises, for 45-60 min. on each of these days. Body weights were moni- tored daily from the beginning of pretraining through the end of the experi- ment. Under 23 -hr. food deprivation the rats were quickly reduced to 80-90% ad lib weight, as in Experiment I.

Experimental procedure

Prior to the beginning of experimental training, the subjects were randomly assigned to either of three groups, Gp CRF (3 rats), Gp CRF-S (3 rats), and Gp PRF (4 rats). Gp CRF received continuous reinforcement throughout acquisition, i.e., every run terminated in reward in GB. Gp CRF-S also received continuous reinforcement throughout acquisition, and differed from Gp CRF only in that discrete environmental stimulus

"w

.

* ' .41'“ i •'M-'®.'? i ® ii ..AC T' .

: e-a , jut's

■: ■■■'?' y i*x nvu ;’iwA':

\: ^ r

m

^

' '■’■ '^'"‘ '" "***•

.rt .* Sit.'Nrf It -I !-.•.' t»i*|,t24*4

At,'t !,=» o '4|lt(jj['.(lf

* i> ^'!< tb.nAl

, .. . i .-)} oy, >t% ■.■£i -•x'tai* tj|;i

'-* 'H'

v*^ m- t .)’ ' ““-a- .--

-m.

L'isJ, i

. b!-'

■s

h y .1 tit: <•>■:. -I;.

,‘;Af£.i

"If:

I !?’>'.'■

^•J

_-:

|w^>«ri:‘£#E(X3^

y4-*wr-.i II I iiiyw^itT®ii[(){|;|Wii*iri.

frTH|||M|i

. ' ' '■ \ !. r* iZi ti

' ‘(Si '■

•Sjh

" .,-»•'•••• •/,'*«/(, xT> . ^ iAA-0^' icijl’

' ' -.') . "Ip

m

-■ jfi

Mti

f. I . 'fy /;/>:• iTt ••." <*»/♦..;••♦*.» tV*:L- V -i ,* t ■' J I

wiW'j.

■■'W;

f

im^

VW>- -.'y-.

89

changes, unrelated to reward, were presented on blocks of trials from trial 12-47, as shown in Table 11. No diffuse olfactory stimulus com- parable to the insecticide spray in Experiment I was used because of pos- sible lingering effects on the behavior of rats in the other groups. Gp PRF received two initial rewarded trials, then 50% partial reinforcement for the remainder of acquisition, with trials rewarded (R) or nonrewarded (N) in the following repeating sequence RRN RNN NRR NRN.

Subjects were run one trial on experimental days 1-3, two trials on day 4, and three trials per day from day 5 on. On days when more than one trial was run, the intertrial interval for a given subject was 12-15 min. The procedure in running an experimental session for a given group was as outlined in Experiment I. Gp CRF was run at 2:30 p.m., Gp PRF at 3:45 p.m., and Gp CRF-S at 5:15 p.m. The daily running order of the subjects within a group was randomized. After a session each subject was fed the difference, if any, between the amount he had received during that session and the amount given on three rewarded trials. Ten min. after this feeding in the animal colony, each subject was given 1-hr. access to food.

Each group ran 59 acquisition trials. On each rewarded trial sub- jects received 12 .045 g. Noyes pellets (.54 g.) in a food cup placed in GB out of view from the alley. On each nonrewarded trial (Gp PRF, and all groups in extinction) the empty food cup was present in GB. Subjects were kept in GB 30-40 sec. on N trials, which was close to the overall mean

1

■■ ■■ / .1 , ! *

„-4» » 4 f T

90

T able 1 1

Extra Stimuli for Gp CRF-S

Stimulus # Trials

Description

1

2

3

4

5

6

12-17 Complex GB stimulus; a black Plexiglas panel replaced the end door in GB, a similar black Plexiglas panel was clamped to the inside of the back wall of GB, the food pellets were placed on a black rectangle of sheet metal on the floor of GB instead of in the food cup, and a cross was made with masking tape on the outside of the clear Plexiglas top of GB. All these stimulus changes occurred in the short side -section of GB and were thus not visible from the alley.

18-23 Overall light level stimulus; the incandescent

fixture was turned off, and overhead fluorescent lights were turned on (alleyway illuminance >

10 ft. candles).

24-29 Alleyway floor stimulus; the floor of the entire apparatus was not scrubbed with Lysol after every run and dried as usual, but was washed with clear cold water and left damp.

33-35 Pre-run auditory stimulus in SB; after the rat

was placed in SB a 10 sec. tape recording of the squeals of a rat being held by his tail was played (80 db). The usual 70 db masking noise was off.

36-41 Complex SB stimulus; SB floor was covered with a ridged black rubber mat. When the rat was placed in SB a small (2 wt. ) light bulb on the outside top of SB began blinking off every 0. 75 sec. in synchrony with an audible "click. "

42-47 Aj object stimulus; a metal tray, 30.5 cm long and 2. 5 cm wide, was suspended from a wall in (from mid-Aj to the beginning of A2) 6.4 cm up from the floor.

I ,■ 'V

! ■/

--it t .

■Mil

'W'"

^4h '

)]*/•*. "i t

IWi ■t'

■JIB*-

iKii-

tr"-.

1 s. f-'M*-.' ..:> _•• I A-

^»,,'ci‘^

-k<-;-y/o

? .>^^■' ., ,,5 ".'. M ; H *:

' 5 . , , , ■'# ' ■';'

,■>' ' ..* .*■ ' aifevi: " ■•V, .-;-v .'.^^

'■ ^‘■im-

’) n

v{tf|

>». ; t , , !i. , '

4»> iV’.-'Tx.r/*, •,'!

fr ■. v.SIfeS

"'i- •'■ •■♦ % ■>,»

\‘ r HfS - f .,.'■{■»•,■/ ai/f

' :i' ’A'

P'

It ^

W;'3

I'i' (‘'.f : . ' A''. .'■'

i -"i , ' -r..,!C" '-; _'" ^ '

■i t^'. im> ^'ii '.V.

.W'-i

-a ■■*

,• ' ^:.

-n

. _.,, - JIV -

%:^/:' .Z3,

‘•V va.^::»'<< ‘M',

■r-A

4#|-

. .■.,,tM :■

nv^

t -- JL

Wy

^lfM

■ii

■1

«ivS5'

P

91

time to consume the pellets on R trials.

Following the 59 acquisition trials, each subject received 18 extinc- tion trials. Other than the absence of reward on every trial in extinction, there was no change in the procedure from acquisition to extinction. At the end of the 6-day extinction period, the subjects of Gp PRF received further training. Rats 210 and 211, who had shown very little decrement in running across the 6 days of extinction, were given 24 more extinction trials across 8 days. Rats 204 and 208, who had shown highly erratic and xmstable performance during acquisition, were shifted to CRF for 24 trials to check that their acquisition performance was in fact due to the PRF schedule, and not merely to individual idiosyncrasy.

Data analysis

Run times for each alleyway segment were automatically recorded, and each trial was video-taped. Analysis of the video-tapes was carried out as in Experiment I. No modification of the classification system pre- sented in Experiment I was required. Trials 1-6 and 19-65 were analyzed independently by a second observer trained in the use of the classification system, but with only rudimentary knowledge of the experimental condi- tions and no knowledge of which subjects were in which conditions. Agree- ment as to whether a given behavior was competing or accessory was > 95%, agreement on the locus of occurrence was > 99%, and on specific categories was approximately 95%. All disagreements were resolved by

-I

m (f

‘WS' r **

. ;4fl^(SW4

'4

.'» I

u

■»-^1

1

.-?* ' ' 41 '

♦»<#?■»*• 3

,1

■■ '-ifk *T“»n^.. rf,i*:i'|i^ i V ,• *3^ i

■‘- f 'aV < Hv’r "^j^fi|f.vi^^, ViK

, ..Mi vii,vi'»‘.^

' ■" V ' A?': it#* * 4 i"| '; ty > '*J!liJim

■m7

m

<vr

^ :i,m

»f. ^ lf’ii##-l / ' VM' f ,f^

..X

' ■. it.-..v

m

J 3 tii.' " *

:tw:

1=34''

'^S' :’ J:

-'■•V, L,i .-•|,i(»i|4

-i-ja". ,, j-.U

T ; *i~- ...■a..,o ij^:::;: -. ,rf)#.i

r^m

,'. ' - ' . ^ . f

.X'AiW^ ;,

&9:

k <

t >.4J

92

reviewing the video-tapes. Each run for each subject was observed on tape a minimum of seven times by the experimenter, including at least two times after every other run was analyzed.

Results

Acquisition

For the CRF subjects, run time decreased somewhat more smoothly and rapidly, with less trial -to -trial variability than for most of the hungry . subjects in Experiment I. With the relatively large amount of reward received here (12 pellets), all reached a stable DR asymptote by trial 20- 25, showing only occasional CB or AB trials thereafter. Otherwise, acquisition looked like that seen for the hungry subjects in Experiment I. Because of this, and because typical CRF acquisition for this experiment can be seen in detail in Fig. 9 for rat 209 up to trial 17, trial -by-trial acquisition data for Gp CRF are not shown.

Figure 9 shows run time for each trial of acquisition, coded as CB, AB, or DR, for each subject of Gp CRF-S. In addition, the time spent in competing behavior per CB trial, the number of bouts of competing behavior per CB trial, the locus and categories of competing behavior, and the locus of accessory behavior are shown for each trial, as in Experiment I, but here only for one representative subject, rat 209. The numbers 1-6 in the top panel denote the extra stimuli. It is seen that some of the extra stimuli presented to the subjects of Gp CRF-S disrupted running. Stimuli

Fig. 9. Behavior in Acquisition for Gp CRF-S.

209

CB AB » DR

Tria Is

95

#3 and #6 (damp floor and tray) had the largest and most consistent disruptive effects, comparable to those seen for some subjects in Experi- ment I with the insecticide stimulus, and in some cases of greater magni- tude. Long CB trials returned for all subjects, but especially rats 209 and 205, after the introduction of these two stimuli. As is shown for rat 209, these long trials involved Icng-duration competing behavior in mul- tiple bouts with many categories, occurring widespread in the alleyway along with accessory behavior. The magnitude of the disruptive effect in the presence of a given extra stimulus decreased across trials, and as run time decreased the underlying behavioral changes for all subjects paralleled those in early acquisition, as shown for rat 209. Stimulus changes involving the goal box (#1) had no effect, and changes in the start box (#4 and #5) had only slight effects for rats 209 and 205, and no effect for rat 213. Throughout acquisition rat 213 showed consistently less dis- ruption of running by the extra stimuli than did the other subjects of Gp CRF-S.

Figure 10 shows overall run time on each trial of acquisition for the PRF subjects, and a fuller trial-by-trial description of behavior for rat 208. In Gp PRF, rats 204 and 208 showed very few DR trials after nonrewarded trials were begun (trial 5). Their times on subsequent CB and AB runs were extremely variable, with long -duration, multiple -bout, multiple- category competing and accessory behavior occurring throughout the alley- way, as on the first few acquisition trials. They did not reach a stable

Fig. 10. Behavior in Acquisition for Gp PRF.

Trials

98

asymptote even by 59 trials. Rats 210 and 211, however, showed a very different kind of adjustment to the PRF schedule. After some typical early acquisition trials involving widespread competing and accessory behavior, and one or two CB trials just after nonreward was introduced, a minimal disruption, rats 210 and 211 showed mostly fast, stable DR runs, with run times slightly but consistently faster throughout (e.g., X for last acquisition session = 1.40 sec.) than those for Gp CRF (X = 1,62 sec. ) .

Table 12 summarizes for each subject and each group the total amount of competing and accessory behavior in acquisition. Within Gp PRF means are also presented separately for rats 204 and 208, and for rats 210 and 211. Overall, Gp CRF clearly showed the least competing , and accessory behavior in acquisition, Gp CRF-S much more, and Gp PRF i the most. However, the group average for Gp PRF is quite misleading, as rats 210 and 211 together showed fewer trials with accessory behavior and fewer CB trials even than Gp CRF, and only slightly more total time in competing behavior, due to one long CB trial (#22) for rat 210, There was some overlap among the individuals of all the groups on each measure presented here, underscoring the range of individual differences seen in this experiment.

Compared to the continuously rewarded, hungry subjects in Experi- ment I, the PRF subjects all showed an unusually high frequency of com- peting, and especially accessory behavior, in (see rat 208, Fig. 10).

ttWf h t#-. Ml. .T^v-..4>iru il- bfii lUt >hw : •<*

M

’»

L-

. V

.'®,'

Mil

ji-! •"-* ' . . '^ ^'V , ,'5

r‘ -i»^ t»!k ^'^CJiitwi'**’ alii-i'fJ i'''

■'^ I,

.-.■'.■-i.m * -.'Im HU(.: «U>rt'i ••■■vi. T.' ®.

^ ' '' ■' '

I

iJWiCiiStF

;.® *(»^«

A/ ^ f I :'- i;i - ol . .’ft

^Slr

<:, y r .jfl » vQj w-ii .

\,''<f-

' JT] Ml

, '.TS» I

V laM ^ ’O’

..ti I ' Y . '^=':!^^ hjl .._

iv‘ '■'*<- V ••..>■ i.r.4* u,o -:

-[ ’■■" - ^

’li!

•;. - j . i-f jj. o kjIfi/i'.tv.Mj ^u'< # ,^.-

* ■V.e

IfflL

vVL£’i r Ml :UV'

?

')?. ?j(» ..1

t-.i>f 'i i

.A i .*(1^042 myrXp^

'.- -h \ «L

.;^,, •' , , ,/' : ' ^ n

-

99

Table 12

Amount of Competing and Accessory Behavior During Acquisition

Subjects

# Trials with AB

# CB Trials

Time in CB (Sec. )

Gp CRF

201

10

14

54. 90

203

16

12

83. 56

206

12

5

10. 95

X

12. 7

10. 3

49. 80

Gp CRF-S

205

20

25

199.99

209

18

22

215. 75

213

7

14

62. 23

X

15

20. 3

159. 32

Gp PRF

204

48

42

497.99

208

46

30

119. 63

X

47

36

308. 81

210

8

7

86. 82

211

10

8

19. 32

X

9

7. 5

53. 07

X

28

21. 7

180. 94

100

This behavior almost always included slowing or stopping and sniffing the floor or walls. The high level of competing and accessory behavior for Gp PRF can be seen in Table 13, which summarizes for each subject and each group the proportion of trials throughout acquisition, given com- peting or accessory behavior, on which competing and accessory behavior occurred in each alleyway segment. Note that the A^ bias of competing behavior was very weak for Gp PRF, and that accessory behavior was in fact strongly biased to A^. In Gp CRF, rats 203 and 206 showed the strong Aj^ bias in competing behavior characteristic of the hungry subjects of Experiment I, However, rat 201 did not, and both rat 201 and rat 203 showed much competing behavior, especially hesitating or stopping and sniffing the floor or walls, in latter parts of A and A . Further, the

Cd C)

CRF subjects as a group showed accessory behavior in A^ about as fre- quently as they did in A^. These results were not seen for the continu- ously rewarded rats in Experiment I. The subjects of Gp CRF-S all showed a consistently higher frequency of competing and accessory behav- ior in Aj^ than in A^, as in Experiment I, although rat 205 also showed a rather high frequency of competing and accessory behavior, usually involving floor -sniffing, in A^. These results overall suggest some influ- ence of the PRF subjects on the behavior of some of the concurrently run, continuously rewarded subjects.

The similar patterns of competing and accessory behavior in latter parts of the alleyway, characteristic of the PRF subjects and some of the

« » » f

)> Til

-i ..o

ff' •» ■■

.< 1 . jn .O. iu 1‘4-V vJ i ' I 'iO

.aJW

; 1 •,’wnej'v- i fL.-AV’ * ‘^‘ f®® 4<- 3^^<f

- 'm

la

« ' <T '

ij

^ ! at-*'\ V#

J. . h f.iii ,^KH -lO .tfi.

fr

tr-t ^T; t, H re ^

* L

® a -

'Ipiai

, SI '.^'-'k-...

eiii'i'^If^jfr'.-l^tfri. ft* v*if«»^<f ;^ ■■,;■■ II? aA|a ^A

y.

, fOn hiiS f*'*f .^sv5t>wb0 .?

r i ■' 1

■'yg:

m: 1

\i r-< I-4V/ '’.f- tiWZf

^ . ■' n ' K ^ W*'

: ^ ^ H ;j'«i jjl' ix?''*-' I . 'A ' iiao'-i ' ; i P> '■» '"%$ '<L^Dv‘ _ /;||

. . . J

. ■.•/• V.-' .f*'r '^r. .,(11 .,A lit y*.

.#•

■i 'm'T #i

./"Vis'

■'v.

■' ' •- (

‘^-r ;ti *tA , ...A «i i|i«4J., ■■'trlioi

( •*!! w ^ - -‘I.

' ri. 4f p-yy -.. c •> w|T*

X ..

v:

- ’■* ^

. ..,« nai: :

' " ft*

/ : . 1 ILI 3 •' h oXf-t't ■h- .»•■» tn iteOp T| ‘|i

’^Jj «. ^.:,--

. . 1 .-y-, > . 'S’ » ttNll^tsC.! f rrti ' I'T 0.^,

'■^ ‘-i*

■:.-r:j«T

•.II. b>i>lta<Maaiaf|>|L N

^ . ., r . ^

101

Table 13

Proportion of Total CB and AB Trials in Acquisition on Which Competing and Accessory Behavior Occured in Each

Alleyway Segment

in Experiment II

Subjects

CB

(AB)

CB

^2

(AB)

CB

^3

(AB)

Gp CRF

201

.43

(.30)

. 50

(.50)

. 36

(.50)

203

. 92

(.88)

. 58

(.63)

. 42

(.50)

206

1. 00

(.50)

0

(.08)

. 20

(.67)

X

. 78

(.56)

. 36

(.46)

. 33

(.56)

Gp CRF-S

205

. 72

(.70)

. 36

(.50)

.44

(.30)

209

.95

(.72)

. 55

(.72)

. 09

(.50)

213

.93

(.71)

.29

(.43)

. 21

(.14)

X

.87

(.71)

.40

(.55)

.25

(.31)

Gp PRF

204

.79

(. 65)

. 26

(.31)

. 38

(.85)

208

. 67

(.48)

.40

(.74)

. 50

(.76)

210

. 71

(.63)

. 14

(.38)

.43

(1. 00)

211

. 50

(.30)

. 63

(.40)

. 50

(1. 00)

X

. 67

(.52)

. 36

(.46)

.45

(.90)

102

continuously rewarded subjects, were not a simple result of behavior "sequencing, " as described in Experiment I. Table 14 shows for each subject and each group the proportion of CB and AB trials clearly involv- ing intra- and inter -individual behavior sequencing. Also shown is the proportion of sequencing trials involving A^ sequencing. It is seen that there was little inter -subject sequencing at all in this experiment, as in Experiment I, and that few of the inter -individual sequencing trials in- volved A^ patterns. High levels of intra -individual sequencing were seen only for rat 201 of Gp CRF, and the highly variable rats 204 and 208 of Gp PRF. Of these subjects only rat 201 showed high levels of intra- individual sequencing in A^. As these data suggest, most competing and accessory behavior in A^ as well as in other parts of the alleyway did not appear in recurrent patterns either within- or between -subjects, but occurred intermittently, in "isolation. "

Even when no competing or accessory behavior occurred for the PRF subjects, their run times were relatively slow in A^. Table 15 shows for each subject and each group the mean run time in each alleyway segment averaged across all DR trials in acquisition. It is seen that in Gp PRF rats 210 and 211 failed to show average A^^ times longer than A^ times, and showed by far the longest run times in A^. Rat 208 showed only a very slight positive goal gradient on the average, and also showed the longest DR times in A^. Only rat 204 showed a large and consistent DR goal gradient. However, almost all of this subject's DR trials

»

lii >/f<» tip, <■■*«* ti>' , |;

t. . . >. a I fv r ^ ' N » ' » ' fr

.*■ '

»fl'. M.i-' ui:K-i;v-r. (Iif-M.i r./i'l >- *- , i»-' x(0.'

1 sti

1.. :! ' ;>/ P>t

.'>*«iifj(i ,i>

,t1

,'-J(»S.“>ii ;1fl ti li.'> -.v J.,4* O-'i

'I tTjt»f.ii»i'' rti

rvivcsrtff’ //Hf-. i; .-'•li

.■-•■ j .■ .-.: .ui

•'. ni'-./uie ^

': ,. ^’ ' ■■■C

' Mi': JhtiJ!;

■•'i f r li-l:

M!.*A •■' V'ft'.iV' ik'V-'

' -r:^ '• ' £ l> ■(.•'i. ' '4» . y- 'JUi "^:^m

! t - t •_*

■lirl jv'-'Uial i'tU. &

liTrlfM.

n, U i tc 'Xcmii.Q Tg &4

103

Table 14

Proportion of CB and AB Trials Showing Intra- and Inter - Individual Behavior Sequences, and Proportion of Sequences Occurring in A^, During Acquisition in Experiment II

Subjects

Intra - Overall

•Individual

(A3)

Inter - Overall

•Individual

(A3)

Gp CRF

201

. 59

(.70)

.06

(1.0)

203

0

-

.05

0

206

0

-

0

-

X

. 20

(.70)

. 04

(.50)

Gp CRF-S

205

. 14

0

.04

0

209

.08

0

.08

0

213

. 13

0

.06

0

X

. 12

0

.06

0

Gp PRF

204

.70

(.24)

.04

(.50)

208

.60

(.20)

.06

(.33)

210

0

-

0

-

211

0

-

.08

0

X

. 33

(.22)

.05

(.28)

104

Table 15

Mean DR Time (Sec. ) per Alleyway Segment During Acquisition in Experiment II

Subjects

^1

^2

A3

Proportion Ai > A2

Gp CRF

201

. 61

. 58

.69

(.65)

203

. 65

. 58

. 66

(.88)

206

. 61

. 56

. 66

(.83)

X

. 62

. 58

. 67

(.79)

Gp CRF-S

205

. 71

. 71

. 81

(.47)

209

. 63

.55

. 56

(.89)

213

. 60

. 52

. 57

{. 84)

X

. 65

.59

. 65

(.73)

Gp PRF

204

.93

. 74

.78

(.80)

208

.91

.89

.96

(.56)

210

.43

.44

. 60

(.48)

211

.43

.43

. 60

(.56)

X

. 68

. 63

. 73

(.60)

105

occurred early in training, and not under partial reinforcement. Again, some of the continuously rewarded subjects showed results comparable to those of the PRF rats, viz., marked slowing in on DR trials. While all the rats in Gp CRF showed a decrease in DR run time from A to A

X u

on the majority of their DR trials, all showed the longest times on the average in A^. In Gp CRF-S, rats 209 and 213 showed a positive goal gradient on a high proportion of their DR trials. But rat 205 did not show an average positive goal gradient, and showed by far the longest times of all in A^. Again, these results were not seen for the hungry, continuously rewarded subjects in Experiment I.

The relative frequency of each category of competing behavior in acquisition for each subject and each group is summarized in Table 16.

The categories of gross movement and receptor -orienting competing behavior are listed in order of their overall frequency in Experiment I (as in Table 8). Categories occurring only after extra stimuli were intro- duced at trial 12 for Gp CRF-S, or only after nonreward was introduced at trial 5 for Gp PRF, are separately marked. Rearing and sniffing at the tray in A^ (stimulus #6) for Gp CRF-S are not shown. The range of cate- gories seen was the same as in Experiment I for each of the groups, ex- cept that Gp PRF failed to show rearing in the center during acquisition. Also as in Experiment I, the most frequently occurring categories of com- peting behavior were stopping and hesitating and sniffing the floor and walls, and the rank-order of all the categories for each group varied little

106

1

1

1

j Table 16

Proportion of CB Trials on Which Each CB Category- Occurred During Acquisition in Experiment II

Subjects

i

ST

Categories of Competing Behavior Gross Movement

H E T 1/2 T 1/4

ret

F

1

Gp CRF

1

201

. 57

. 57

. 14

0

. 14

0

203

. 75

. 67

.25

. 08

. 08

. 08

206

i

. 60

. 20

. 20

.40

. 20

0

i _

1 X

.64

.48

. 20

. 16

. 14

. 03

Gp CRF-

S

205

. 80

.44

. 12^

. 16^

. 04^

.40

209

. 74

. 73

. 26

. 30

. 04

. 13

213

.79

. 36

. 07

0

. 07

0

X

1

. 78

. 51

. 15

. 15

. 05

. 18

i Gp PRF

204

. 67

. 50

.40

. 24^

. 19^

.29^

208

. 70

. 60

.27

. 13^

0

0

i 210

. 75

. 38

. 13^

. 38^

. 13^

0

211

. 75

. 38

. 13

. 13

0

0

X

1

. 72

.47

. 23

. 22

00

o

. 07^

Category seen only after introduction of extra stimuli (CRF-S) or

nonre-war

d (PRF).

107

Receptor-Orienting Activity

S(F)

S(W)

S(C)

S(P)

R(W)

S(DW)

S(T)

Fa (r, 1)

R

(DW)

R(T)

R(C)

1. 00

. 50

. 36

. 14

. 14

0

. 14

0

0

0

0

. 83

. 75

. 75

. 25

. 33

. 08

. 17

. 08

. 08

. 08

. 08

1. 00

.40

. 20

0

0

0

0

0

0

0

0

.94

. 55

.44

. 13

. 16

. 03

. 10

. 03

. 03

. 03

. 03

.80

.40

. 28

. 08

.24

. 08^

. 16

. 04^

. 04^

. 12

1

0 ;

1. 00

. 52

. 57

. 13

.39

. 04

. 22

0

. 04

. 13

. 13

.79

. 50

.29

. 36

. 07

. 14

. 14

. 07^

. 07

. 07

0

.86

.47

. 38

. 19

.23

.09

. 17

. 04^

. 05

. 11

. 04

. 83

. 71

, 31

. 10^

. 21^

.31

. 07^

. 05^

. 26

. 02^

0

. 83

. 83

. 37

.37

. 16^

. 03^

. 20

. 13^

. 03

0

0

. 50

.75

. 50

CO

.25

. 38

0

0

.25

0

0

.75

. 88

. 63

0

. 13^

0

. 13

0

0

0

0

.73

.79

.45

. 15

. 19

. 18

. 10

. 05^

. 14

f-H

o

0

i

108

from that seen in Experiment I. The relative frequency of the more com- mon categories varied little on the average from group to group, with the exception that the subjects of Gp PRF showed a consistently higher relative frequency of wall- sniffing and somewhat less floor- sniffing than the sub- jects of the other groups. It can be seen that there were no categories associated exclusively with extra stimuli or with nonreward, although some minor categories occurred for Gps GRF-S and PRF only after these changes (Fa (r, 1) for both groups, and T 1/4, F, and R(T) for Gp PRF). Each category shown by an individual subject only after a change in conditions was usually shown by others in the same group before the change, and was in any case shown by subjects of Gp CRF, not experiencing either extra stimuli or nonreward in acquisition. As Table 17 shows, the range, rank- order, and frequency of categories of accessory behavior were also very similar for the three groups. Floor- sniffing and accessory hesitations were by far the most frequent, with sniffing in the center occurring some- what less often. Also, Gp PRF showed somewhat more accessory wall- sniffing than the other two groups, and Gp CRF less sniffing in the center.

It is seen that little accessory photocell- sniffing occurred, none for Gp CRF, and none for the other two groups before changes in reward or environmental conditions. Too much emphasis should not be placed on this observation, however, since photocell- sniffing in competing form occurred in early acquisition for subjects of all groups. It is of interest that, while every category of accessory behavior seen here was also seen

St ,

r_i-

fj^:d. it ^i||Hi»« 14-HkJ

’■f i

' ■- _

h:M.

M

; , ..■•».♦'*> V <■ r I I'J '1^'-'

^ ^ \a \ wa^irpAtl ■'

:>'4

•i'r. .. vf la'4ia&4L

- .j* ' . ' ■'^i#'.'*|pl

N'i* "1 flt*5 i.>‘ f'''f ^ 3v'i '^ut>V jCf H '• ^ti> ^''t^fXK,' .^'4^,■S.^!t.

_ .■'3> ■■■-.'. '

'A*-. ;•> •»..» t7W i.!j>- 1 >iI Tt.n» ,»!»•»•»»

1

> l.*' '^■' V’i 4 ii^ ■^^'.:>i

t.

. '. -• i '

-. (’■•• •■ iw-j ■uifu*^ -vAi XI 0 1 'J■^^♦0/Yi^f*p||M fi

'■i^. “ri. ' ‘.‘

. %.

i3vi»rc

r-t?^

s * i ail ^ii

-0 •••<>-- i-.v

, .,: *rrn\( I IS -'i(M-*r'< .,?t,t(i'f-«l) >-H<

. f '• -

■' < ' ' . '. ' is

1 If

u( •/'itiiroo !r.'i.»n Hs 1 h<l i;q , ,o«^;- .,

I ^ 0 .*. -' i- - ' .-.'M^j-r-.* D,v<‘l

.^f. ^ #;,- ^..1^ i_^ ■»■ ■»

I .-. ; ^ : * . I V 1* - 1 'i q t' .:

JW

■«

,1 1 11

•1

■I®'

t ’• /[ i;- »itM/n I ii>H . ftiSt-'SIlifettAiia ' t ,

- •■ , .. S"V'J ' « ■'■'

; nji-t ip- *■ c ■' -f a y it i 'v -&9,rm

II

-'s oVtiv/ 4

•-.' ' ^' *' 'Hi

'/" f.

"■- j^'r^ ^:-

vi . .'*'.jj

109

Table 17

Proportion of CB and AB Trials on Which Each AB Category- Occurred During Acquisition in Experirnent II

Subjects

Categories of Accessory Behavior

Gross

Movement

Receptor -Orienting Activit-y

AH

S(F) S(C) S(W) Fa(r, 1) S(P)

Gp CRF

201

. 50

.90

. 10

0

0

0

203

. 50

. 88

. 19

0

. 13

0

206

.42

.92

. 08

. 08

. 17

0

X

.47

.90

. 12

. 03

. 10

0

Gp CRF-S

205

. 35

. 80

.20

. 05^

0

. 05^

209

. 72

. 83

. 22

. 22

. 06^

0

213

. 14^

. 71

.29

0

0

0

X

.40

. 78

.24

.09

. 02^

. 02^

Gp PRF

204

. 35^

.98

. 08

. 10

0

0

208

.47^

.85

. 17

. 32^

. 23^

.11^

210

. 38

. 88

. 38

.25

0

0

211

.70

. 60

. 20

. 20

. 30

0

X

.48

. 83

.21

. 22

. 13

. 03^-

^Category seen only after introduction of extra stimuli (CRF-S) or nonreward (PRF).

110

in Experiment I, in approximately the same rank -order, the range was narrower here; four of the least frequent accessory behaviors in Experi- ment I were not seen at all in acquisition here (R(W), R(C), S(T), and R(T)).

Finally, a period of slightly longer run times for several subjects was seen in acquisition, at trials 50-56 (rats 201 and 203, not shown; rats 209 and 205, Fig. 9; rats 208, 204, and 211, Fig. 10). As in Experi- ment I, this increase in run time, corresponding to the occurrence of competing and accessory behavior, was not a function of overt behavior sequencing.

Extinction

Figure 11 presents a description, plotted as in previous figures, of the behavior of each individual in Gp CRF during extinction. Run times for the last two acquisition sessions are also shown. Points representing run times on the last trial of a given session and the first trial of the next ses- sion are not connected, to facilitate assessment of the "spontaneous recovery" (Pavlov, 1927) of run time from one session to the next. It is seen first that long run times returned after a very few nonrewar ded trials (2-4). These long trials invariably involved competing behavior, and usually accessory behavior as well. The competing and accessory behavior returned first in the latter parts of the alleyway for all the CRF

subjects, in A for rat 201 and in A for rats 203 and 20 6, before 3 ^

Fig. 11. Behavior in Extinction for Gp CRF.

NO.CB

Behavior category and locus ^outs CB duration Run time (sec) > > > (sec)

113

spreading quickly throughout the alleyway. The long CB trials character- istic of extinction closely resembled typical long runs in the earliest stages of acquisition. As can be seen for each subject in Fig. 11, these long trials showed long-duration competing behavior in multiple bouts throughout the alleyway, with multiple categories occurring on each trial. Also, much accessory behavior occurred throughout the alleyway.

Table 18 shows the relative frequency of the categories of competing behavior and averaged for each group for the 18 trials of extinction. The order in which the categories are listed is the same as in Table 16. Cate- gories seen only in extinction are separately marked. The range of cate- gories was the same as in acquisition for both continuously rewarded groups; Gp PRF failed to show three minor categories (Fa (r, 1), R(T), and R(C)). Also, the rank-order for groups overall was quite similar to that in acquisition, and in Experiment I, with only minor exceptions, e, g. , R(DW) for Gp CRF. In Gp CRF some subjects (rats 201 and 206) showed categories of competing behavior in extinction which they had not shown in acquisition. However, they were not "new" behaviors, peculiar to extinc- tion (e.g., emotional or frustration -elicited behaviors). They were all seen in acquisition for other CRF subjects. Accessory behavior in extinc- tion (Table 19) also did not differ markedly from that in acquisition for the CRF subjects, though there were some minor changes. Thus the fre- quency of accessory wall -sniffing increased in extinction. Also, the range was extended, in that rat 203 showed accessory photocell- and top -sniffing.

4

Table 18

Proportion of CB Trials on Which Each CB Category Occurred During Extinction in Experiment II

Subjects Categories of Competing Behavior

# CB Gross Movement

Trials

ST

H

E

T 1/2 ret

T 1/4

F

Gp CRF

201

15

. 80

. 87

. 33

. 20

. 07

. 07

203

14

.79

. 64

. 21

.29

. 07

. 21

206

11

. 82

.64

.45

. 36

. 18

0

X

13. 3

. 80

. 72

. 33

. 28

. 11

.09

Gp CRF-S

205

13

. 62

.46

.31

. 38

. 08

. 38

209

12

. 75

.92

. 17

. 08

. 08

. 08

213

13

. 77

.69

.46

. 31

. 23

. 08^

X

12. 7

. 71

.69

. 31

. 26

. 13

. 18

Gp PRF^ 204

12

. 58

. 17

. 58

. 08

.25

. 58

208

13

. 54

. 54

0

0

0

0

X

12. 5

. 56

. 36

.29

. 04

. 13

.29

Category seen only in extinction.

Rats 210 and 211 showed no CB trials during the 18-trial extinction period.

115

Receptor -Orienting Activity

S(F)

S(W)

S(C)

S(P)

R(W)

S(DW)

S(T)

Fa (r, 1)

R

(DW)

R(T)

R(C)

. 87

. 67

. 33

. 67

. 33

. 13^

. 07

. 07

.13^

0

0

1. 00

.93

.43

. 36

. 58

.43

. 21

. 07

.43

. 14

. 14

1. 00

. 73

. 18

. 18^

. 18^

cL

.27

. 18^

. 09^

. 18^

.09^

0

.96

. 78

. 31

.40

. 36

. 28

. 15

. 08

.25

. 08

. 05

. 77

. 31

0

0

.23

0

0

. 08

0

0

0

.92

. 50

. 67

. 17

.25

.08

.42

0

. 08

. 17

. 25

.77

. 77

. 38

. 69

. 31

. 15

. 08

. 31

. 15

. 08

. 08

.82

. 53

.35

.29

. 26

. 08

. 17

. 13

. 08

. 08

. 11

.58

. 58

. 17

0

. 08

. 17

0

0

. 17

0

0

.92

. 62

.54

. 08

0

. 08

. 15

0

0

0

0

.75

. 60

. 36

. 04

. 04

. 13

. 08

0

.09

0

0

Rats 210 and 211 showed no accessory behavior during the 18-trial extinction period.

117

not seen for any CRF subjects in acquisition. However, these categories, and other categories seen for the first time in extinction, were all shown in competing form in acquisition.

Figure 12 shows extinction data for the individuals of Gp CRF-S, plotted as for Gp CRF, and Table 20 summarizes the mean run time for both groups of continuously rewarded subjects in extinction, in blocks of 3 trials. The onset of long trials in extinction was slightly slower for Gp CRF-S than for Gp CRF. Mean run time across the first 3 extinction trials for Gp CRF-S was 2.79 sec., and for Gp CRF was 6.22 sec. Also, run times greater than 5 sec. returned after a mean of 4, 67 trials for Gp CRF -S, compared to 2. 67 for Gp CRF. There was overlap among the individuals of the two groups. Thus, as can be seen in Figs. 11 and 12, while rat 205 clearly showed greater initial resistance to extinction than any CRF subject, rat 201 (CRF) was comparable to rat 213 (CRF-S), and showed greater resistance to extinction at least across the first 4 trials than rat 209 (CRF-S). Run times for the two groups overall were com- parable from the second to the fourth block of extinction trials, but on the last two blocks of trials Gp CRF again showed longer run times than Gp CRF-S (Table 20), due largely to the very long times for rat 203 (see Fig. 11).

As with Gp CRF, once running began to break down and long run times returned, trials for the CRF-S subjects looked like those in early acquisition, with long -duration, multiple -bout, multiple -category

Fig. 12. Behavior in Extinction for Gp CRF-S.

Tri a Is

120

121

competing behavior, and accessory behavior, occurring throughout the alleyway (Fig. 12). For rats 209 and 213, competing and accessory behav- ior returned first in or A^, then spread quickly throughout, as for the CRF subjects. Competing behavior also returned first in A^ for rat 205, but for this subject competing behavior occurred much more often in A^^ than in latter parts of the alleyway. However, accessory behavior was as widespread for rat 205 as for the other CRF-S subjects. There were no novel competing behaviors in extinction (Table 18), and no accessory behaviors which did not occur in acquisition, at least in competing form (Table 19). Also, the range, rank-order, and relative frequency of the competing and accessory behaviors were much the same as in acquisition. The frequency of accessory wall -sniffing increased, as for Gp CRF, and accessory R(C) occurred for the first time, for rat 209. Still, two cate- gories of accessory behavior seen in Experiment I did not occur here for either continuously rewarded group (R(T) and R(W)).

There are two further points of interest about extinction following continuously rewarded acquisition, with or without extra stimuli, which can be seen clearly in the individual data in Figs. 11 and 12. Five of the six subjects of Gps CRF and CRF-S showed some spontaneous recovery of running on the second session of extinction. Trial 63, the first trial of the second day of extinction, was a DR or AB trial faster than the preced- ing trial (usually a CB trial) for all except rat 209. Further, for all of these subjects except rats 203 and 206, a comparable spontaneous

T i?i; S" , '^(Bi ' 'i.j '4*1 U-* Siife VOij, ,W;So“ t>^ ,* (&i l»WY^

f.- ’W/''‘ ^ . ,^„

.-^MU[| A VO t toij

It T j . ) ' '. j ; *“’ U *i

-/ -LKwpflHH

- ^ ' "JE12

< •' . ^• -t" ^’■

*'V »vrv<^

» ' 'I?

ft >3..)Tlft^'

•t.:“.- .i3.*r:-T '•••6 ,(^U 4,f rfOijWi;/!-?

' ': ' ^" i'“ 4 - ■'

.^Hk, ;■ 'V.

r*' '

■^a: ■■ , .

*ii*> gVUi'./nt brr^ , t r»i,v-o-'’liyiVfi .•;;,( -

10

) ' .•''',

1.19

.;. -i- , A-'X* -ww ,'TL

. £SiHfl ' ^

I.w ,lJ3xJ -4^ |oi an ,'■ •-: > -1 :>i:ri ' •’*v' 1 , Ui f? ' >-■ i o. . >•■ 1 . t

'•-^ - ;., "V ^

\^„ J; " ^

nr 'I .1 (.;•> t ; .• Lij -: iM, '

■‘ 'll y: '" ‘t ^ ^

' ,;, / .■ ®;JSa '■ jfl W^

,. . , i.l-iOLV&> iW.» . A.yCt‘h-

, ! L..-'V' o:

I ;'• I l»'jt

■, vr‘iv/ ■'

*i. j|«f7

>t Ii' -.'/o;'c

TO^^O.

I I i .noitr'/ri 1 <.,. tt<. fejt*ana<.,|||il ■-.. viii^piUlP

'O ‘t. ■m-

-1 ' ^

•W'^.:- ' > -'irf

? ni*'^

122

recovery effect can be seen on the third day of extinction, between trials 66 and 65. Spontaneous recovery may occur still later in extinction, as can be seen for individual subjects across the remainder of extinction. Second, it is seen in Figs. 11 and 12 that for individual subjects extinction following continuously rewarded acquisition appeared to be a cyclic, non- monotonic process, independently of any spontaneous recovery effects.

Run time typically increased abruptly from 3-4 sec. or less to 20-25 sec. or more in 1-2 trials. These long CB trials were followed by much shorter CB trials, or less frequently by AB or DR trials, singly or in runs. Since the cycles for different individuals did not show the same periodicity and were not exactly in phase (Figs. 11 and 12), averaging times across sub- jects obscures the effect, making the increase in run time in extinction appear more smooth and gradual (see Table 20). While long extinction trials did look like long early acquisition trials, shorter extinction trials after the initial breakdown in running differed from shorter trials later in acquisition in one important respect; in the absence of the selection for running, a high frequency of competing and accessory behavior was still seen in on both the long and the shorter extinction trials (as on shorter PRF acquisition trials for rat 204, and especially rat 208, Fig. 10). This can be seen in Figs. 11 and 12 (e.g., rat 201, trials 66-74; rat 206, trials 71-77; rat 209, trials 76-77; and rat 213, trials 70-71).

The behavior of the PRF subjects in extinction, shown in Fig. 13, looked like their behavior in acquisition. Rat 210 showed no decrement in

Fig. 13. Behavior in Extinction for Gp PRF.

CRF Ext. CRF

8

Trials

i.... '1

125

running across the 42 nonrewarded trials; all of the first 18 extinction trials and 23 of the subsequent 24 extinction trials were fast DR trials.

Rat 211 showed slightly more variability in run time than was typical of acquisition, and showed a few fast CB trials after about 20 nonrewarded trials, but fast DR trials still predominated throughout the 42 extinction trials. Rats 204 and 208 continued to show the overall variability, and the corresponding high frequency of competing and accessory behavior which

I

had characterized their performance in acquisition. Overall run time |

I

increased slightly in extinction for both of these subjects. Rat 208 showed !

I

a strong bias in competing and accessory behavior in extinction. Rat 204 showed a similar bias in accessory behavior, but competing behav- ior was most prominent in A^. No novel competing or accessory behaviors | occurred in extinction for the PRF subjects (Tables 18 and 19). In sum, for the PRF group a,s a whole the decrement in running during extinction was very slight (greater resistance to extinction), and there was essen- tially no decrement for those subjects who showed the least disruption, and the fastest, most stable run times, in acquisition.

During CRF reacquisition following extinction for rats 204 and 208 of Gp PRF (trials 78-101), run time and overall variability decreased (Fig. 13). After a few CRF sessions, relatively fast and stable AB and DR runs predominated, suggesting strongly that the erratic performance of these subjects in acquisition was in fact due to the PRF schedule.

The high level of competing and accessory behavior throughout the

<ini3 * ^ f

. »f .i.1 -.;>•.■! nUM|^

i».‘' 'J

i>o4‘ 4l4iT^’

I.

V- .

U -j’diiilSife 11.‘ M .'4:i'S.

' - fc -- : .

«

E'^ -ff' '

•t'-j ! i : ' •. i.-. ,';■ .^vtATf

*;.i**‘J *li ?

•■ . . . V 1

tsi e,9UHii |^< I

■■ 1 ; 0 -^O- Ufii. ^

^ ^ I . C,'

ft*

•3J ti-.-^iiriJ \6,(iJ i I-' IcO

r > ' , V- -I ■J^ftJSSTj >JS

/tH.< -V -*■•'*6 ^ -..11 - M

. ■•a'ii 5-1.

■J c

' n-

.'• <■' ^ '■ - *T^I. <W ■- :jBf

. .*^4* ^ isr^^x ...

•'f=“--''^ ... ^'?|i

vv^' ,^^^V f^'i. A T' -■‘.^•it-^liv.--

frii-r'K '*'. ' -;i -t .i( fa*.>/'?)U i *< 'Jt A^^Ii

2s.-

jji

%■

0

:- 'i' i ' *. .i.'orrjc rt?;» •^t -/£jyti;:jf. :^:«'3:,:^.-»^^

.- tiv.i :iui h'^-->'-:-: r-ffvv A a u

' , s .'^7 ^

a oi)T v-^-r

- . j, ' f

^ .aW. r. A!k.i ' '' '

. 1.2 T

ri»'

lilui >1*.*^ *

, •- ‘I'^m

Itch I ' V --■ ri\>i i^44v JM^^' in '-'■'^

. . - 7 ' Iftf

« -t . t « I

< s ■( . i T ,

^- -^n4;>' i^%.y»w:- f- ; *rri

-■:. -.jj

.m:

TflL'

, «| ;., .y

' .-:i4

*' s#- |l

'M

1

126

Table 2 1

Proportion of CB and AB Trials Showing Intra- and Inter- Individual Behavior Sequences, and Proportion of Sequences Occurring in A^, During Extinction in Experiment II

Subjects

Intra -Individual Overall (A^)

Inter - Overall

Individual

(A3)

Go CRF

201

. 19

0

0

-

203

. 38

0

.06

0

206

. 13

0

. 13

0

X

. 23

0

.06

0

Go CRF-S

205

. 60

0

. 07

0

209

. 38

0

. 19

0

213

.20

0

. 07

0

X

.39

0

. 11

0

Go PRF^

204

1. 00

(. 17)

. 06

0

208

. 83

(.20)

.11

0

X

.92

(.19)

.09

0

^Rats

210 and 211

showed no competing

or accessory

behavior during

extinction.

127

alleyway in extinction for all subjects was associated with a relatively high frequency of intr a -individual behavior sequencing, as is shown in Table 21. The frequency of inter -individual sequencing was still quite low. For most subjects the frequency of both kinds of recurrent sequences increased from acquisition. Of the behavior sequencing which occurred, very little involved sequencing, as in acquisition.

Discussion

The results of this experiment show that the great increase in run time with the removal of reward following continuously rewarded acquisi- tion in the alleyway reflects the rapid return of competing and accessory behavior, and not a simple decrease in the rate of running (Bindra, 1961; Estes, 1950, 1959; Guthrie, 1959; Marx & Brownstein, 1963; McCoy & Marx, 1965; Miller & Miles, 1936; Miller &; Stevenson, 1936; Wendt,

1936). For both Gp CRF and Gp CRF-S, this noninstrumental behavior returned first in the latter parts of the alleyway and spread quickly throughout. This is in contrast to the return of competing and accessory behavior with reward magnitude downshifts or an extra stimulus, which generally occurs first and most prominently in as shown in Experi- ment I. Consistent with the present extinction results, Bruce (1935) found that, when food reward for hungry rats and water reward for thirsty rats were switched, "exploratory" behavior returned first in the latter parts of a maze before spreading quickly throughout. For both groups of

f

t I

^ I? J

1 ^

■’■lie 0/01

..HVi't t I '

'.*• ii’’ ,,

■1^

^■zf,- . -jt'’ f>wi

i.’tr I O'-

-■^, i' ' '

t "* -TT^

I .. :• -f.o

. , ’1' •>

4- ".■'“ .

.' »f .•••■.■ ijj.rli ■■,

, ji-f. i •... H"! »; M 1 ;u a

-•■ riAi-;;?**. inq-B'f ^tU p''r/M»n*%

’■'■■ k, -iii

*. :»sk^

•. rt W.i J.'t>.:^«^. ■8*e«TSi^ ;HSi'1^Jft ' »

•a r-i ^ /

/ ' ; -J' '■‘’ , JO ■: ■:

.• . i.i:? , •*•- ■' TC' qO ,5.rt.5f ^rCKl\'llUEi^^

. 1 r ' - t

.- .■/ 1^.-^ ( I i . : w ”:1': bvj(: ‘f 'Ji3i^4*' 2|t?'

■'■'■' --^ R- 'JR» 4JSI

■'• :' ,

. .. .J J,'.. ■•’ ..T» 0,1 *o fsJ- Wt ^■■'

,V-i^> ^ .,- ,/ V

1 .. "",■, ,■■r•■f

. '1 I'ii'j.^fwuj’ i? ,u- :. I ' ■;.

•/ L.! ro- . umo-iq.:4‘=?'3/V» '"

, J- .. - ’^>1

. r ^ i'-' A •» Tifa(d^'r<^

ai‘

, i^i£|ELh

ifiv/,. loii .-'X' V fiorf

' V . ^'' V r "

^ -- 'i . _.;■■■/ ' .;?.'j:5i#

' ; *i ' ' ■/ ■•

■■■,«(

"£dl

**«cm A

128

continuously rewarded rats, the categories of competing and accessory behavior seen here in extinction were the same as those seen in acquisi- tion, though individual subjects sometimes showed a wider range of behav- iors in extinction than they had in acquisition. Also, the rank-order and relative frequency of the categories of competing and accessory behavior generally varied little from acquisition to extinction. Thus, the return of competing and accessory behavior with extinction, as with other experi- mental manipulations, appears to reflect a stable, organized hierarchy of variable behaviors from which behaviors seen initially in training also derive. It seems clear that competing and accessory behaviors in extinc- tion reflect neither a simple within-subjects "regression" to earlier behavior patterns (Miller Stevenson, 1936) nor the elicitation of "novel" behaviors specific to nonreward (e.g., frustration-elicited behaviors, Amsel, 1958; Spence, I960). These extinction results are consistent with the general view of the alleyway as a selective learning situation outlined in Experiment I.

The prediction that continuously rewarded subjects exposed to extra stimuli in acquisition which engender competing behavior would show greater resistance to extinction received some support, Gp CRF-S showed slightly greater initial resistance to extinction than Gp CRF, due largely to one CRF-S subject, rat 205. There was no consistent difference be- tween the two groups later in extinction except that the CRF-S subjects again showed somewhat more resistance to extinction across the last two

rr.a ^

TTT

<t u r '

v^rt *- 1

1 1

^

4 ' I -

,«l H«tf

< . 9

' -/■

u '! y- fy y»i J :f^ j p,i mu

^ . v>^

, '■' ",\ Q

. \

^. I’f ' ' ;j£.

\ ^ . ., a

, '.'4 ,fi.7 M v^a.r’. 1 izt,/i'’ ': i **x.

.. ' ^ .: - 'I'Wi

' .■ , I ' jir,’rrro 3 ' >}, 'frioa 4^

' f 1 (

...

. iv .. v;,rr». - >.J '.TejMfttaj

' '%y S

1*1 '

9-r .*i.',D£»1 'gf:! jfL* -■>

i ,- I

* ^4^'

« ' M- 1 u/l Jrr» :i)< •//. i'itxvti'^pd •'( -tAc^ o^X tll/ns

. "'^1 '' (, r^i^S ~

^!j .'>■'■ •''■ .' 4.J-.vd

•!.!'■ " T :t- Iji

vH^?i fi.

./ . i ! . .',. . D1 :' I/t:] . »a) &*S^V^.

'■' '*■■■ ^ ■'"^-^1 .V . ■( * ■^■'

.= i ■'

1 ^ «iiiA

'■

. .f?<i! -vvJ: i-'-i fit 4?

,?-t f|ft/t •,;.^.i- ••»

. 5'. 1 : ■•■ ~ fi B-.f,

4v/:iin^Vixi '

*" , .rV-t 4 .*. 7 '

- M ‘‘' ^‘ 7'' " - -

•* l<: G*t'c :• A I ;,iaai.’t -:1

* . M ■■' m,, r'jp "

VjrfT '

■'”•■ . ®^-,’

.;■ _ •■■y^

1 7 *'

'■ -^ ^ ' ii

*

r'- 4 - u'

i-- -v.'*« -». 7 . - . iii 4M

^ '

o f I.: ’W i/'-iij

129

sessions, mainly due to the extremely long run times for rat 203. Though these results suggest, consistent with other alleyway results (e.g., McNamara & Wike, 1958; Robbins, 1971), that inducing competing behavior in acquisition may be sufficient to retard extinction, the relatively small extinction differences between the two groups do not seem commensurate with the great differences in the amount of competing and accessory behav- ior in acquisition.

It is clear that the extinction of running following continuously rewarded acquisition (Gps CRF and CRF-S) is a nonmonotonic process; after an abrupt initial increase in run time, much faster trials returned, singly or in runs. Spontaneous recovery from the last trial of one session to the first trial of the next, seen here to be a reliable, single -subject phenomenon, did not account entirely for this cyclicity of run time in extinction. This cyclicity has been shown repeatedly in free-operant situa- tions (cf. , Ferster &: Skinner, 1957, pp. 52-63, 346-351, 411-413; Horns & Heron, 1940; Skinner, 1933, 1961), though rarely observed in the alley- way (see Miller & Stevenson, 1936). This reflects the greater focus of free-operant studies on single -subject behavior, albeit automatically recorded. Given that the cycles of individual subjects have different peri- ods and are not exactly in phase, the usual practice of averaging run times across subjects in the alleyway obscures the effect.

Partial reinforcement here had a bi -modal effect in acquisition.

Some rats (204 and 208) showed much competing and accessory behavior

S3 ,

* ^.j\-.f, i ' -,fMS U.rra* !jr^> - - ^ 3«|<3 tU*'^

' ^ tf . A

fi

!«•

fij . <lt.-

. ( s-v l»*

!»'^V

>n I>i;. o:ii »<J>13ri||i‘>ft,c^.?rtl^^^ f“Ji

.. ..y, ... .*

r ' . V|pi=^ ,

,'('.' ()*■ ^ n '^t

. {.. , U j^ii f v/.-i I Jw.f I u ,uwr I'o ncrJt^mili^ .^5^%4u .- j

•,.7jo.,..>*.-..*^.rj li- St (3-|t);5Iv'V fc,.^»j

i -•*>... V. . . ^.t,

. ; »f- '.r»i .-- j / I ^ ^

^Bi' . ;■'• ,3

3

If,;' . ■■•? |j.;r - V*' ;,^ , , i. v

' ' * '' ^ "P di' •' V _

i: - ' fi

'. I .i . . ! ^ •!

m'.

^ . . I . .'■■4,® vf

w ' tMtk

#£5 ■....' .;;.)^| . ■^,.

li 9 Til-' ^'.’

' ^ _■» . I

-*.'.’*3i:! ivtJP-

•,. . , f -.v . uif* - J.V.'- ,3^T;isct^. ,

' 1.31'

•-..* ' S~ ' '.' '"-‘W4

'm.

■T-.'.iV

... ^ !

.V-jr-;.s?r''i >j-' 11:' i ?i /T

s : 1.1K .iife'

.’ t S '

, s.-At,. - •.‘.^- •r.fWi

1 vvs \o 7 icu*'Hi/ '«t’J xVi . . t ■• M' '•

^. » P 3 f < r <- y t .v> /• 5 ■-' .‘I I

II

130

throughout acquisition, especially in latter parts of the alleyway, and did not reach a stable asymptote of DR runs even after prolonged training, comprising 59 trials across 22 days. In CRF training after extinction these subjects showed markedly less noninstrumental behavior and in- creasingly stable run times, suggesting strongly that their erratic per- formance in PRF acquisition was indeed due to the PRF schedule and not merely to individual idiosyncrasy. Other subjects (rats 210 and 211) showed little competing or accessory behavior in PRF acquisition, and quickly reached a stable asymptote of extremely fast DR runs. If this split proves to be a general outcome, it might help to account for the in- conclusive results with PRF acquisition in the alleyway literature, with some studies reporting PRF groups running slower overall than CRF groups at asymptote (see Kimble, 1961, p. 315; Lewis, I960; Lewis & Cotton, 1957), some finding no difference (Goodrich, 1959, Exp. I; Wein- stock, 1954), and some finding faster asymptotic running for FRF groups (Goodrich, 1959, Exp. II; see Robbins, 1971; Weinstock, 1958). Differ- ences in the proportion of PRF subjects showing each kind of adjustment to the partial reinforcement schedule, revealed only through an analysis of single -subject behavior, could easily account for the variable results.

The vastly greater resistance to extinction of the PRF subjects did not depend on partial reinforcement engendering competing behavior in acquisition. While PRF can produce a high level of competing behavior, it does not inevitably do so, and the present results indicate that the PRF

A;.

»#*t » l- #i .

', -.»•'♦!« f).-“vti.

•:. ,»('r^rvv ioti’

|5 <i 7' '

> •’ r \ I .' I I

r--

^ 1 4 i<. •■ l>

,l«p -i"

■• tti' ,«4«JilE??C<l«<l4;

a ’^. t"

. ■' ,. ■' ...

' ' ■' ^ ' 4 -C’ - .

I, ;-j di^iOJ^CjiTCj?

■:! 7»'..Y;*^I

"'

If I (

fc.i.«Vu ^

*.: ' .. 'i'w©-: .-i

■i ' ;, ■' '"’5f

•-• •■■nUft ' f'- 2: <• Vi^k^ftOl ■.

' . ■'•<>• ‘yi - :fr

\t ri

V?''.'-' ' : ., i.'.l»'.(^#l «5&e 4i '’!

,mm hM^ -v-r -I Iijj^rl*:.-.^

n u*. < y (r .lA Y iakrorj }

-'"V

> .. ■i.*i;<'S*oj' 'y'^lauaWfe 'V^’j^

"^ ' , ' . . i?'' .

" i V/ •_ Vw*"' fo. a '^.

-■ * ' ' . I . M -ijg £h» 3 ; ff* j V t«;' * SCT

S3I i^iaar

4#^ ■:

I V.

M ^ ,.'iTii|a«"

’• ..r,-n,3ji..,'^i ^ j roll'll ‘rt'r yt>l% IX><J -Jv-tdij

131

subjects who showed the least competing behavior in acquisition were the most resistant to extinction; rats 210 and 211 showed only about as much competing behavior as the subjects in Gp CRF, yet continued to show fast DR runs, with no decrement in run time, even after 42 nonrewarded trials across 12 days. Thus, the expression of high levels of overt competing behavior in acquisition may be sufficient for greater resistance to extinc- tion (e.g., Gp CRF-S), but it is not necessary.

A recent account of behavioral "persistence” in extinction (Amsel, 1972) predicts that the more a subject comes to run in the presence of a "disruptive" stimulus, the greater his resistance to extinction. This account assumes that it is not the amount of competing behavior expressed in acquisition, but the extent to which running dominates in the presence of disruptive stimuli such as occasional nonreward, which determines resis- tance to extinction. This kind of account has been applied to the partial reinforcement effect before (Estes, 1959; McNamara & Wike, 1958; Wein- stock, 1954). It fits the present PRF results quite well. However, this view should predict that the CRF -S subject who showed the least disruption of running throughout training with extra stimuli (rat 213) would show the greatest resistance to extinction. In fact, rat 205, who showed a large- scale disruption in acquisition, showed by far the greatest initial resistance to extinction in Gp CRF-S.

The PRF subjects showed high levels of A^ competing and accessory behavior and relatively slow running in A^ even when no competing or

'I

u;'*’

3

i:>j

jj 1

«.*TT.,

>* <;■

u

'-,'i']“. 'Wiii

; . ^ c; : > ^ti . ' :»! ,c< i '.ali'V

fl

w

, ; * ' f , J rt v"\ J!lla <*

: ,<»aoi: IK

■*,’•* ':w' ii6* s»^ -y^' ' i - » *5', ^ ^ 4

a*

14'

» ^.. j4*wU3si^ ■' '•' ■■' ■■"■ v-3tiip, m

^ ' '■ , '4;' ■/; “•

. a. t»i.ii i^n ii. i,.:d" 'iM-’-fm' iP' .iii' *«i.gt>y

^ <1;

'M.a

rv!?

fjui^qa .. rsjjLtt

.Ijsi.-i -it i '>!..• i f ^-i f'. '£.r.f^

ar«tnittv.n\v£fi'W -vi Vilj jJ|‘J

■T?)

' ‘r^iv

j'w: :vi- :?j (

■■ ,. ' m

o;. <o :->rj.;.-: ^‘/tt

* V s

^ ..> ' . : , 'm'^s

B

■J !? ,^9 %

•4,; :,g.-

. * ■• s ,*4.’.,, ;).;|t/,.fA

■..'V'v'!

132

accessory behavior occurredo Some continuously rewarded subjects of both Gps CRF and CRF-S, unlike the hungry subjects of Experiment I, showed behavior with very similar structure. These apparent behavioral interactions were largely independent of any overt inter -individual sequencing. Previous experiments have suggested similar interactions at a very gross level, in terms of slowing of overall run speed near the goal, between control subjects and subjects receiving nonreward or reduced reward (Ludvigson & Sytsma, 1967; McHose & Ludvigson, 1966), In addi- tion to such interactions between the partially and continuously rewarded rats, the observation of single -subject behavior in both experiments re- ported here revealed recurrent behavior sequences between subjects, and occasional periods of long rion times for several subjects, not involving inter -individual sequencing. Although these interactions all appear to involve odor tracing, especially since floor- and wall -sniffing are so prominent, and communication via other sensory systems can be virtually rilled out, their chemical and physiological mechanism is not at all under- stood, Overall, it is certainly clear from these observations that the behavior of an individual rat is not entirely independent of the behavior of other rats run concurrently in the alleyway.

There is evidence that the structure of behavior observed here in extinction was not an artifact of these poorly understood between-subject interactions. Pilot experiments running individual subjects through CRF acquisition and extinction by themselves have found the same results re- ported in detail here (Kello, unpublished observations).

M ku((jde I til l t: ' -'it’jTii >C»

f r.

.1-

.-4r%

♦W'

W|vli<<^c{ h^warf^

.- m '

'•- '> '•fljai 1 1 ■'»Viv' ■‘-'f f> ' «i.- > 1: r0 ,<■• odi- ; ■: .:

I ,. : I

••R ' /t‘d 'd

»r *

\. !Q uTW'^iib

•;T/ a

t*. rtuT - i£> .• ,

■■ -■'■ ^ , ,'^ ,

•'■ ''' 'J4 Tlao.>

•■ -i& ^ iy

n\ Ivc:-. . l.'-va .-'i v>4'-»W .'M. •< *

‘rri fv*

l‘

. -k *tij '.',n..s'cf* icsgsdi.^,

?■■

'r.*V5kv.»!i •■.

- . 1 1/; j<t-'tt|».Htfi >.i£

H m^'dhoHo^" , ,

I , . T-' ^ 'H

r. . Irtl/U -.nj.4 i|^'i?t». i^'0 »'<^^

-3>l«r.v i./

'lf%'

,Tl<"

tn . r .^ . fi > tft y >' ■^ *’ ^ ■? 'If .fifpv '^tir,p: ^

' -i ' ■' ■'■ iS' ^ 'r^. . •-»..

t .

tr.ifJiJ 7 'J a7::ii ill

j. -.n. iW' :<_.‘';rt ’, lr»ii'aui l6|:

.■>' ■'^1

^■J n i .1 r.^:

►i>odJi».

7U

'! . Irt . :>iiJ ,

iV

li ItiSl .4^

"r" ^

, JT _:i k'

'■C ^ ' i ■■■

; .orn- y!V &>c^ 'Vq ’■• j|^^ m& A<3|-^^

•'a»*<i";S' i**.uti y '•tM'r--*»^f I » '/.,• >i,i^T'^ i4£/'Qilyi*^T9^43l ^i,

. - .: /5 .,

iy.C' f.4'-^'’,;A,:. f

v.it* '»•»■';'>'>

H-^ft Ot -‘l^f jtgC^Q^I^, J

T^i e«4>~

REFERENCES

Amsel, A. The role of frustrative nonreward in noncontinuous reward situations. Psychological Bulletin, 1958, 55, 102-119o

Amsel, A. Behavioral habituation, counterconditioning, and a general theory of persistence. In A. H. Black & W, F. Prokasy (Eds.), Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory. New York:

Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1972.

Bakan, D. A generalization of Sidman's results on group and individual functions and a criterion. Psychological Bulletin, 1954, 51, 63-64.

Baum, W. M., & Rachlin, H. Choice as time allocation. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1969, 12, 861-874.

Bindra, D. Components of general activity and the analysis of behavior. Psychological Review, 1961, 68, 205-215.

Bindra, D. Effects of nonreinforcement, drive -diminution, and punish- ment on relevant and irrelevant behavior in the runway. Psychological Record, 1963, 13, 273-282.

Bitterman, M. E. Toward a comparative psychology of learning. Ameri- can Psychologist, I960, 15, 704-712.

Blough, D. S. Interresponse time as a function of continuous variables:

A new method and some data. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1963, 237-246.

Bolles, R. C. A replication and further analysis of a study on position reversal learning in hungry and thirsty rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1958, 51, 349.

Bolles, R. C. Comments on Professor Estes' paper. In M. R. Jones

(Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation. Vol. 6. Lincoln: Univer- sity of Nebraska Press, 1958.

■24,

'fr- . r 7 ) ft 7. ■■ i r^.'M;- •Yl'li?T'.t^i%> (' ''r-4. ,f*t ,y^, ,:'h ‘^4*

! -iiOr

r.C

'®: ,. . '■'

' * ’'C2?' ' " *' ' * -i-' ■' " .^,v^'*>-3 i';>^* '•'' ' ''

-• ' ' .. j(!^hfh '"• < > > . KB 9fi 1''-* ,' ija *M',

^ -

■'■-.nr-- .••i-i/>' ,rrif>ft-5;0s0Tij-'::..i',^^

!■ -V . 4-.

. \ iiij^ ^ ^ ,i .;

w.V^' l'^ A %»*»■*>'«

. . i * . . . ^'

!* W-, •''"!■

. .*<{• '.ti .• G j 6 , -i v'^v^ii-'>.»*r^#''^if,i^|S

■' V w .. -MM

^ . ..-• '■'#^' .- ^ - . {.^v:

L' >■' .'v-i^.

'(^ T '-'.hutXif ^

I.-

. i/^.' ( z*i

:#/t>jfr'^ h(td"

W:-v

,;'^\ii#»fl9a./ ',1 ■■^tfoidpjh ''■‘j:. '’I

sfS',

Spw<y'i'M»i<Bgia|^‘'» -

134

Bolles, R. C., & Petrinovichj L. Body weight changes and behavioral attributes. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology,

1956, 49_, 177-180.

Bower, G. H. A contrast effect in differential conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1961, 62, 196-199.

Brown, J. S. The motivation of behavior. New York: McGraw-Hill,

1961.

Bruce, R. H. A further study of the effect of variation of reward and drive upon the maze performance of rats. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 1935, 157-182.

Bruce, R. H. An experimental investigation of the thirst drive in rats with especial reference to the goal gradient hypothesis. Journal of General Psychology, 1937, 17, 49-62.

Campbell, B. A., &; Cicala, G. A. Studies of water deprivation in rats as a function of age. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psy- chology, 1962, 763-768.

Campbell, E. M. , & Meyer, P. A. Effects of daily reward sequence on simultaneous and successive negative contrast in rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1971, 74, 434-440.

Catania, A. C. Behavioral contrast in a multiple and concurrent schedule of reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1961, 4, 335-342.

Cicala, G. A. Running speed in rats as a function of drive level and

presence or absence of competing response trials. Journal of Experi- mental Psychology, 1961, 62, 329-334.

Cotton, J. W. Running time as a function of amount of food deprivation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1953, 46, 341-357.

Courtney, R. J. , Jr., Reid, L. D. , & Was den, R. E. Suppression of running times by olfactory stimuli. Psychonomic Science, 1968, 12, 315-316.

Craig, W. Appetites and aversions as constituents of instincts. Biological Bulletin, 1918, 34, 91-107.

Crespi, L. P. Quantitative variation of incentive and performance in the white rat. American Journal of Psychology, 1942, 55, 467-517.

£ . . V. ■*'.**

,1i .•j'llv.

•■TH ;

47:' -"Jv.'

■■■■ t5* ^

|fciSaS-'ii»* ills' j?fl'

Vi

iff»y W!»i^ ^ ;».i.',.«t! 'K^/ V ,S , '. , i7Wi3'5^?

^ V .-,— ::rr‘ '^■

'ii

•ijAi Vi

m

V** ■* i .'. V 'if^l v^*i*Tl® -aj

r>.; (<■■ .A . IV'

,,•• v/1 ■> W .*tiS}.M2>iTu:|^^

> •? M”^.'

='■■ %,

i ^ ^

. .V ^ , t *i;i^- , ; 4?:7i^H;^i3rriS ^

Ml: . , V '.t ’^1

. >:y.:.^.-‘ .,^%3'I^^'V

V V^-' . ' J

‘- - it kifniP t' ' ' n «i :-/ , )>

"'• * ■•'•• - 5'V .t ':iS ^ ’’1^

'^i.

; , Vj V

ih

I » I 1

ili ,i‘5.;v - , V J:

. . U' >V- /ift iA-ij v-:rr'v fc'y’a t. t4^: £‘jtlivf , ji f

- -■ ' ■-. . '■ ffi , f:

•r

./r-Mp; ■‘'^- '■

Jl ■'• \:lm^

..ii •iflirii^ tii"'<M*

135

Estes, W. K, Toward a statistical theory of learning. Psychological Review, 1950, 94-107.

Estes, W . Ko The problem of inference from curves based on group data. Psychological Bulletin, 1956, 53, 134-140,

Estes, W , K. Stimulus -response theory of drive. In M. R, Jones (Ed,), Nebraska symposium on motivation. Vol, 6. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press, 1958.

Estes, W. K, The statistical approach to learning theory. In S, Koch

(Ed.), Psychology: A study of a science, Vol. 2. New York: McGraw- Hill, 1959.

Falk, J. L. The nature and determinants of adjunctive behavior. In, Schedule -induced a.nd schedule -dependent phenomena, Vol, 2.

Toronto: Addiction Research Foundation, 1970,

Ferster, C. B., & Skinner, B. F, Schedules of reinforcement. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1957.

Gagne, R, M, External inhibition and disinhibition in a conditioned oper- ant response. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1941, 29, 104- 116.

Gilbert, T. F. Fundamental dimensional properties of the operant. Psychological Review, 1958, 65, 272-285.

Goodrich, K. P. Performance in different segments of an instrumental response chain as a function of reinforcement schedule. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1 9 5 9 1 57, 57-63.

Graham, C. H,, & Gagne, R. M, The acquisition, extinction, and spon- taneous recovery of a conditioned operant response. Journal of Experi- mental Psychology, 1940, 26, 251-280.

Gray, J, Animal locomotion. London: William Clowes & Sons, Ltd.,

1968.

Guthrie, E. R. The psychology of learning. New York: Harper, 1935.

Guthrie, E. R. Association by contiguity. In S. Koch (Ed.), Psychology:

A study of a science. Vol, 2, New York: McGraw-Hill, 1959.

in.

I

A : '■:*''

I.:,

. '^T ■ir.Si'>W _ .*»oiuS

«: ; SS'^' ’’'' Scifk

ri art i'

.1

If! ■-; &•-

V* *' ’■ |MI"‘ '

'^* - 'J* ■‘‘^ ' ?

■*^-i> . ffll '’'^ '

■-'■'A n 't ify, = : v_^ ''^ ii'*’ U'-* ^

■■ ' ^ .P^ft

ar- ' ' '■ '■

' .i ? i- '-wq*"- ' '• * » .•■'■

'-*• ' * ftp f?Aj t alE^

S . ! '. (- .<i^acir

,3' ■•'■«

"■ ' I fc '>■'*'■'■ -■

VI tf,, TO'.-^

: ri- -?i ••|■•>■*e »t

i,7.jy.Uy y tM'C.yyL

■— jH*!"

,..|w5

'iSt,

,'j 11 if '■■ »i 14 ' '■< iB ' *1^ ,'T ,i

V f. .1 I : * •« t

."ti ,jWJ

r> *•

\ ■> - ' rjrr. _ , I .. ' < r -Irfe

. t •>. TV' ^' '•:' f

1

'n s'Sl g:*M;

.V

“I 4 Jr.jcifjKf'''.! Pp4tldl5tl^)*j>A 7 «4£--

H- . J.K

136

Hall, J. F. Activity as a function of a restricted drinking schedule.

Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1955, 265-

266.

Hammer, L. R. Reinforcement magnitude effects with overtraining. Psychonomic Science, 1971, 22, 295-297.

Hayes, K. J. The backward curve: A method for the study of learning. Psychological Review, 1953, 60, 269-275.

Hicks, V. C. The relative values of the different curves of learning. Journal of Animal Behavior, 1911, 1, 138-156.

Hinde, R. A. Animal behaviour. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1970.

Hoffman, H. S., & Overman, W., Jr. Performance disruption by startle- eliciting acoustic stimuli. Psychonomic Science, 1971, 24, 233-235.

Horns, H. L., &: Heron, W. T. A study of disinhibition in the white rat. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 1940, 30, 97-102.

Hull, C. L. Simple trial -and-error learning: A study in psychological theory. Psychological Review, 1930, 37, 241-256.

Hull, C. L, The rat’s speed-of -locomotion gradient in the approach to food. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 1934, 17, 393-422.

Hull, C. L. Principles of behavior. New York: Appleton -Century- Crofts, 1943.

Jenkins, H. M. , & Arnold, L. Autoshaping of pigeons with food and water reinforcement. Film. McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario, 1968. Cited by Shettleworth, S. J. Constraints on learning. In D. S. Lehr- man, R. A. Hinde, & E. Shaw (Eds.), Advances in the study of behav- ior. Vol, 4. New York: Academic Press, 1972.

Jones, E. C., & Bridges, C. C., Jr. Competing responses and the par- tial reinforcement effect. Psychonomic Science, 1966, 483-484.

Kimble, G. A. Hilgard and Marquis' conditioning and learning. (2nded.) New York: Appleton -Century-Crofts, 1961.

King, R. A. The effects of training and motivation on the components of a learned instrumental response. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Duke University, 1959.

t

■SJ'L

> K' - t * '■ ■< r . . IX' > •' i- f liiti '

-'/j ;; “:'I

■,!Jj i*E(IOliv

I •'

j.V,' «

■U ' fi j* >' -A Tt 1 # ' ■<< I i <.*i, *

! ■? '. n

r. -‘V^? A?

- ' ' ■?teXi . :i

. '■ 1^ J - ^ , ^ I 'ii-' ' ' W 5'-s5^

r.:: .; ; ^ ■'-

, I - _ai , >-■ / ■:*<

^ ".£tanni :%

<n ...

M

, ),. -1o X W _»••.' tr»?^ sJ' 4.p**

fame^U'

'. 'ny ' ■'"‘ •w y .P^

f * ' V

■jj w' IV -kusfji

»mqmT»-.dr xpy'iMtuH

' ■^‘■' I rv t»f' ^..■'^f■'^

'•* -i : -3. gft; ' fij

n^tt ,»nq«3fe.';a'.,

P3JJ V ^

7^;. M ,

. ** >.i -iF»K ,*i r'T?Mfr':0 ■V’^/'*''

, '• 1 ,' ,vt V'-'r ••^' ji'2li

'3

..lii-.ft,

r--; - -.T:/«-|vrTO£‘ ' i>l<r , •/

Ip^' ' ^ •X-' ti

!>\’*

IJ* «*.. ',

■•(', W--

< ' o ! V '.

ftdll '%v if'

g.

137

Kintsch, W. Runway performance as a function of drive strength and

magnitude of reinforcement o Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1962, 882-887.

Knarr, F, A., &: Collier, G. Taste and consummatory activity in amount and gradient of reinforcement functions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1962, 579-588.

Levy, N. , & Seward, J, P. Frustration and homogeneity of rewards in . the double runway. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1969, 81, 460-463. ^

Lewis, D. J. Partial reinforcement: A selective review of the literature since 1950. Psychological Bulletin, I960, 57, 1-28.

Lewis, D. J. , & Cotton, J. W. The effect of intertrial interval and num- ber of acquisition trials with partial reinforcement on performance. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1959, 52, 598- 601.

Logan, F. A. The Hull-Spence approach. In S. Koch (Ed.), Psychology: A study of a science. Vol. 2. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1959.

Logan, F. A. Incentive. New Haven: Yale University Press, I960.

Logan, F. A. , & Spanier, D. Relative effect of delay of food and water reward. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1970, 72, 102-104.

Ludvigson, H. W., & Sytsma, D. The sweet smell of success: Apparent double alternation in the rat. Psychonomic Science, 1967, 9^, 283-284.

Macdonald, G, E. , & de Toledo, L. Partial reinforcement effects and type of reward. (In preparation) Cited by Shettleworth, S. J. Con- straints on learning. In D. S. Lehrman, R. A. Hinde, &; E. Shaw (Eds.), Advances in the study of behavior. Vol, 4. New York: Aca- demic Press, 1972.

Mackintosh, I. Irrelevant responses during extinction. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 1955, % 183-189.

Marx, M. H, , & Brownstein, A. J. Effects of incentive magnitude on running speeds without competing responses in acquisition and extinc- tion. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1963, 182-189.

i|4

,-SiS -'VT^' i$^ . V;^^‘ f jV§5' V |X'

B

r».'^. < 7;r*f»-‘>a^..ir/vU io»# -w 1; 4-.5^

.'-ti*"’* .JVilli:'''*'''*'' * a Tip ”1 ‘- ’'ntr

.._3f " 5

i).

V »

«;r'

‘f 1 r V

' ^ »Yatoi> f -^i

* . ' .. ^ ■*■■«. ■■.^■g"*- ' ". -■ SI'*' -*' -i'-*

^ ^.y, . 'j?' .

* •■' ’*' ; >;^,',-p -■ r . r _ ' oT's^i^

,--i .a'O!/ ' *^t; •,(■!-'

. '^lio V .V ,. f ?

1 \ -ratw ",)’-4 ' , ■:\^Sfi '■’-

' i>ji' <.vtr^:w,K' ;»■' .,t 2 ^a£l^d^

' '■ i.'-’ /W‘--/' i> <'.■■■' «V'J8'#.- . -r->a»Uv\ y vlfex4iv<*^l-y^ ^ }i;

' ' ' ' Y

■i.- i . li a>t'^v*4 '.;.flT ;U ^ ^ ’S

^ ii. -tigl^L:' uL?^ - ‘^'- *■ ,'

T ,,' tliy ifffr, T ^ rrabij^ ' '; ,|1^

.!'rr ttc. ^^^.ilr^gi^J'--: ;.io?TsYi v' ^ ^- - V 'il

- ’4’* ' V •#«' . i * ■ar'^fj*-v%-:.t>^. y / 'i *i:Wi

•<W>» ' * I" ''»1Ml^^;;

T,

I * . -^ L

»'* ,' •gUiHi.o-:' »<!#». ^^aa»T iiWr^i'.p'tneS -I

* '‘- , ' t’ '■ .^^. ^^i.; ^ m

■n i:t-

138

McCoy, D. F. , & Marx, M. H. Competing responses and the partial reinforcement effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1965, 70, 352-356.

McHose, J. H, , & Ludvigson, H. W. Differential conditioning with non- differential reinforcement. Psychonomic Science, 1966, 6, 485-486.

McNamara, H, J. , & Wike, E. L. The effects of irregular learning con- ditions upon the rate and permanence of learning. Journal of Com- parative and Physiological Psychology, 1958, 51, 363-366.

Miller, N. E. , &; Miles, W. R. Effect of caffeine on the running speed of hungry, satiated, and frustrated rats. Journal of Comparative Psy- chology, 1935, 397-412.

Miller, N. E. , & Miles, W. R. Alcohol and removal of reward. An analytical study of rodent maze behavior. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 1936, 21, 179-204.

Miller, N. E. , & Stevenson, S. S, Agitated behavior of rats during

experimental extinction and a curve of spontaneous recovery. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 1936, 21, 205-231.

Munn, N. L. Handbook of psychological research on the rat. Boston; Houghton Mifflin, 1950.

Pavlov, I. P. Conditioned reflexes. (Translated by G. V. Anrep) London: Oxford University Press, 1927.

Pereboom, A. C. A note on the Crespi effect. Psychological Review,

1957, 263-264.

Pereboom, A. C., & Crawford, B. M. Instrumental and competing behavior as a function of trials and reward magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1958, 56, 82-85.

Petrinovich, L., & Bolles, R. C. Deprivation states and behavioral attribute s . Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology,

1954, £7, 450-453.

Premack, D. Reinforcement theory. In M. R. Jones (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation. Vol. 13. Lincoln: University of Nebraska

Press, 1965.

1T.I

HU.

t .. ..

•, »toft4rp

.4-'% ,

imn>; <^mt?5fioicUtyi

I ^ »e ' ^ Jiicp'^

. ',iC^' _ '.H .■'.nrfg'-iirt ,.H .t

J.. -:r.vt:)l<> .?T .J

i , .1 .H

,. ,1 < .-:- ^ .♦! lo i‘

. iry

... ~ ■**, •• ' » ^ .r *}•■''■ •■>

. >j-2i*.a -I-' .■■■'■ ,st-'»*'M '

.f '■ 7% ^ 1^ . y

v'- •■ ,,^

. .>' ■•. •? •>» ■■ ’J ^ ’* *^‘ *“"

,?a^ (.■ ^ ' .; ' '*1

-I ' '»a' 3bfi(3^ * '■; Ari .c .Si . *i5 .'fl

I. - ‘“■\tJ -v3

t ' ;,w'-

..... w-.-. ' •">.^‘^'lar%j.. •' /i . w;.' .^iK-;.^., ,i *

lSi^>KOCS

J -J J 7> '* ' ■- ■iK/k'..i *-»arfS» i£,

I-'

m-

,,^ ^.S *•. >

.'A .H '^i .5 "

'^i

. ' r^ V- > : ' ■" '

« '

? * V*

., t - rfewg

^ t ' AU‘TA2;*<^' ’■' j£l^2SS '

^ ..'

1/ Jll Vd" #^,:vJ5yDt¥x*^-

.T.,! .a-wi

139

Premack, D. , & Schaeffer, R, W„ Distributional properties of operant- level locomotion in the rat» Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1962, 5, 89-95.

Prescott, R. G. W. Some behavioural effects of variables which influence the 'general level of activity' of rats. Animal Behavior, 1970, 18, 791-796.

Pryor, K. W., Haag, R., 8z O'Reilly, J. The creative porpoise; Train- ing for novel behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1969, 12, 653-661.

Rachlin, H. Contrast and matching. Psychological Review, in press, 1972.

Rashotte, M. E. , Adelman, L., & Dove, L. D. Influence of percentage - reinforcement on runway rxinning of rats. Learning and Motivation,

1972, 194-208.

Reynolds, G. S. Behavioral contrast. Journal of the Experimental Analy- sis of Behavior, 1961, 57-71.

Robbins, D, Partial reinforcement: A selective review of the alleyway literature since I960. Psychological Bulletin, 1971, 76, 415-431.

Ross, R. R. Positive and negative partial reinforcement effects carried through continuous reinforcement, changed motivation, and changed response. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1964, 68, 492-502.

Seligman, M. E. P. On the generality of the laws of learning. Psycho - logical Review, 1970, 77, 406-418.

Shettleworth, S. J. Constraints on learning. In D. S. Lehrman, R. A. Hinde, & E. Shaw (Eds.), Advances in the study of behavior. Vol. 4. New York: Academic Press, 1972.

Sidman, M. A note on functional relations obtained from group data. Psychological Bulletin, 1952, 49, 263-269.

Siegel, P. S. The role of absolute initial response strength in simple trial -and-error learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1945,

3^, 199-205.

Skinner, B. F. On the rate of extinction of a conditioned reflex. Journal of General Psychology, 1933, 114-129.

T'T T£J!Ki.. .fr < I

-s 1. < ,. r i',\ . - . Ij- it - !

Utu^u ..fCt

■' * ' ^ < yicivtdt'Ct .

i

,H- ,tio ^

. ^.i . ; i T_^ tA : tjlfiijnc.' j^1 . ^ '

>9\-m SM

A 'i

i

...-•T ,t.' v;> \ ^..v- .'A ^-nri

V .g.-:^ #<v\rc .^ -..lit

'.' *J , *. »V* .1 ■. iX , ., in/y*

1*^.! 4»' % - -> ' » •*•'■ r- - 4, •!

[ , * '7 ”■ "t^ ' ,-"■.*-^•*'-'‘”•11^^^

V . r;^n< *'/;.^Vi«.r 'No' 'IjguwaS^^

■:i.^ ■/-, --i;^

: I

■iTj r'i .-

'• '!' fl j_‘-_

. I < v Ht Mf ; V ' . ■. >'. j ^3'.,

u. ■■■' :‘ :-*R.A

4 .

u

'-■* I . J ‘'47c V .'«■ '-■) -lO ,

*r,f ,Tt

rpV'-)

V.;.-- ..

I E^V;*rlA

V ■' 4'^% '1

m

••i!« , 3;-f .j i

if ( ' I

.. »«(i.-,J . s|4 __ j, :■ .;*

I •M'j icil H '>0#

140

Skinner, B. F. The behavior of organisms; an experimental analysis.

New York: Appleton -Century, 1938.

Skinner, B. F. Cumulative record. (Rev. ed. ) New York: Appleton - Century-Crofts, 1961.

Smith, B. J-i. Irrelevant stimuli and response rate for a pigeon. Psycho- nomic Science, 1971, 24, 103-104.

Smoot, K. E. Effects of drive strength and incentive magnitude with and without competing responses. (Doctoral dissertation. University of Missouri) Ann Arbor, Mich.: University Microfilms, 1964, No, 64- 1727.

Spence, K. W. Behavior theory and conditioning. New Haven: Yale

University Press, 1956.

Spence, K. W. Behavior theory and selective learning. In M. R. Jones (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation. Vol. 6. Lincoln: Univer- sity of Nebraska Press, 1958.

Spence, K, W. Behavior theory and learning: Selected papers. Engle-

wood Cliffs, N. J. : Prentice -Hall, I960.

Staddon, J, E. R. Factors affecting terminal and interim activities. Unpublished manuscript, Duke University, 1972.

Staddon, J. E. R., & Simmelhag, V. L, The 'superstition' experiment:

A reexamination of its implications for the principles of adaptive behav- ior. Psychological Review, 1971, 78, 3-43.

Tapp, J. T. Activity, reactivity, and the behavior -directing properties of stimuli. In J. T. Tapp (Ed.), Reinforcement and behavior. New York: Academic Press, 1969.

Teitelbaum, P. The use of operant methods in the assessment and control of motivational states. In W. K. Honig (Ed.), Operant behavior: Areas

of research and application. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts,

1966.

Warden, C. J. Animal motivation studies. The albino rat. New York: Columbia University Press, 1931.

Weinstock, S. Resistance to extinction of a running response following

partial reinforcement under widely spaced trials. Journal of Compara- tive and Physiological Psychology, 1954, 47 , 318-323.

^ ,r ■■

* k\

■■■

,. > i"'P -iL*: *Vi

' ~ ;^»oy

,?3'

.tr#^tM‘iA :.iToY i '

i 1 i* vtj*it

. N . -17 c ! rjwe-T|L|j>j^5^

:'J: r' . » ' »

1 <> ^ f ;'Sffn'ti3^^ww^0 'Sb'” ''■ ' : j

I i'

•'ll ■jpv#yy‘ :v.?£ii:^S4^^^3

.*•:»'! >C. '^lUX

.14

t.n.3fl)juiHS^-,«’

■—■•■—* > .>■■--» -»— - ^ ,• j..,,. ': 2 .".w

, V ;^?>

f'f.

e,< ')-.r

■--I

X t"

' ^ t"'‘ Ifcf ' '1

. . ••r*»i. ■■ '■■ .1. , J .Sf'w *.7l';;;,.^A^^<^i;--v; .'laa

, I » iv = t i% J-pi'-

c -- ' ' "

i4i(«‘I ■-

.*<»?<5]tt>

(:T I . ':'■ , r>"A‘T':iii','«7tj*;^ J<5 ^ . X . ^O^.b.Jii'3^

■V ■: -t&yii I y, ■'^•‘^-'-■■i4-ua^

t-- ;, . V t'T‘ ( . V T . .. .-. 4 :>;♦ ft r.-l>^;>i.ir^ '' , t. t;

rft ...

ll/fV^Rl^ll is. V 5 fc''"*

■(

i.i ...i srii., :»'.• -‘2=»¥rf.* iq ',

/P'fll’ C'

> i*t‘

I 1 t - -

:.. . r ■*

. y ui ;^3'

. 'f--: , J‘-

r;

y

yv.'w.-

.14- 1 nfwJ.i*.

.4 uJ;»' '- . Svj'r? '<’*a •'5? m,'

f . r.

Jj ,r r;**' ,- '• ^\9 if ii

- ij«t; ''V. I- i'.' ' . t.:--.' ■' >Mi» >VJ ^

. . /it •■ . ,' 4 .■ , ' '-a-- ; -/ /- . ; -..VJt: " '.I-

'•il-M, ,Vi, .1,,*. 4 \

141

Weinstock, S. Acquisition and extinction of a partially reinforced running response at a 24 -hour intertrial interval. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1958, 151-158.

Wendt, G. R. An interpretation of inhibition of conditioned reflexes as competition between reaction systems. Psychological Review, 1936, 43, 258-281.

Winnick, W. A., & Hunt, J. McV. The effect of an extra stimulus upon strength of response during acquisition and extinction. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1951, 41, 205-215.

Zeaman, D. Response latency as a function of the amount of reinforce- ment. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1949, 39, 466-483.

Zener, K. The significance of behavior accompanying conditioned sali- vary secretion for theories of the conditioned response. American Journal of Psychology, 1937, 50, 384-403.

Zimmerman, D. W. Rate changes after unscheduled omission and pre- sentation of reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1971, 15, 261-270.

',2 .W

iU*?!- f t

ffim z |

t''‘iry5#isS ■»

ia. r:^ U-t* r -^ ,

)l

'”V> "J

'*.' icitfUfj *.t r

«» ''»ur io;f>r.^U

I * * - * . I i

' . >

'4!

■'■; -..'V

■: ' . ' . .

■1,... ir; 4'.

- ♦" -a ■■‘H ;. r

-^r'

'V' Z . ^ r-jg;S'.'''^ppy^ iL

>• - •}.

'• .'T-i '.■ M'.’:, -ct ■» /* 'k";;*ir"vUu>'i

.■>"i »ct I

,'!.?• t ynfc : ..

O :i iC.^ n" ' "

" i->^ - I- ^

M . I

r

V I i fr'IVrT': ;

r (.■■< * |( t*^ I'o r!*?

Cif.

BIOGRAPHY

Name:

John Edward Kello

Born:

September 12, 1946, Norfolk, Virginia.

Education:

Old Dominion College, 1964-1968, B. S. , summa cum laude, in psychology.

Duke University, 1 968 -present, M. A. (1970), in psychology.

Honors :

Norfolk City School Board Scholarship to ODC (1964-1966).

ODC Faculty Scholarship (1966-1968).

Member, Delta Phi Omega honorary scholastic fraternity (1967).

Awarded competitiye Biomedical Sciences Support Grant from Duke University for summer research, 1969-1972.

Associate Member, Sigma Xi.

Research and Teaching Positions:

Psychology Assistant in human factors laboratory. Naval Safety Center, Norfolk, Virginia, 1967-1968.

Tutor, Duke University, in junior-senior level tutorial on S-R approaches to frustration. Spring Semester, 1971.

Teaching and laboratory assistant, Duke University, for Dr. J. E. R. Staddon: undergraduate learning course, 1970 and 1971; undergraduate seminar in adaptive behavior, 1971.

142

. ', f , / , I .jn*'*'

..lrf^J•^ -o . ij . •'V-; f»iQ

'. i'- i. t ' ' ',y.

' . f i««i’ f I

O'*

'. . >'. j'-.*

I . , A y’>v.';'"-U ^

‘<Vi

■' I - i'.' f

r '.' I) »'

"

.1 ji\V

- i I ; 0’i;j

J '.‘t *

\i-l

142

Publications :

Kello, J. E. The reinforcement-omission effect on fixed-interval schedules; Frustration or inhibition? Learning and Motivation, 1972, 138-147.

Kello, J. E. Response "momentum" on two- valued cyclic -interval schedules. Submitted for publication.

\

;

''i'-

'5