FA ft ABE LER Li BAEEEELAEE 965 cae tee a ee rt 4 2 ay | 4 ; i eee Wyte 2. WORKS BY THE SAME AUTHOR. RESPONSE IN THE LIVING AND NON- LIVING. With 117 Illustrations. S8vo. 10s. 6d. 1902, ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY OF PLANTS, (To be published shortly.) LONGMANS, GREEN, & CO., 39 Paternoster Row, London, New York and Bombay. FLANT RESPONSE AS A MEANS OF PHYSIOLOGICAL INVESTIGATION BY pecs DIS CHUNDER BOSE, M.A.) D.Se. PROFESSOR, PRESIDENCY COLLEGE, CALCUTTA WITH ILLUSTRATIONS LIBRARY NEW YORK BOTANICAL GARDEN meNGMANS, GREEN, AND CO. 39 PATERNOSTER ROW, LONDON NEW YORK AND BOMBAY 1906 da AE cece wert Fae, + 1906 MAY 2 PREFACE new york THE investigations described in the present volume have been an outcome of my work on the Similarity of Responsive Phenomena in Inorganic and Living Matter, first com- municated as a Memoir to the Science Congress at Paris, in August 1900,' and subsequently expanded into greater detail in my book on ‘Response in the Living and Non-Living.’ The electrical responses described in the Memoir referred to, had been obtained by the method of conductivity variation. The same problem was next attacked by a different mode of investigation, response being now obtained by electromotive variation.” Believing in the continuity of responsive pheno- mena in the inorganic and organic, I undertook on that occasion to demonstrate by the same method the electrical response of ordinary plants, and to show that every plant, and every organ of every plant, was excitable. It was then generally believed that so-called ‘sensitive’ plants alone ex- hibited excitation by electrical response, and the proposition that ordinary plants also showed excitatory electrical response to mechanical stimulation, and that such response was appro- priately modified under physiological changes, was much controverted. I have to thank Professor Sidney H. Vines, ' «De la Généralité des Phénomeénes Moléculaires produits par l’Electricité sur la Matiere Inorganique et sur la Maticre Vivante.’ (Zvavaux du Congres International de Physique. Paris, 1900.) * Paper read before Royal Society, June 6, 1901. Also Friday Evening Discourse, Royal Institution, May 10, 1go!. Vili PLANT RESPONSE at that time President, for the facilities which he then afforded me for the full publication of my results in the ‘Journal’ of the Linnean Society, and for the warm interest which he has manifested in my work, both then and later. I next undertook to demonstrate that all the important characteristics of the responses exhibited by even the most highly differentiated animal tissues, were also to be found in those of the plant.' In my previous investigations I had shown that the tissues even of ordinary plants gave electrical signs of excitatory response. I now undertook an inquiry as to why they should not also exhibit response by mechanical indications; and I was surprised to discover that ordinary plants, usually re- garded as insensitive, gave motile responses, which had hitherto passed unnoticed. From the point of view of its movements a plant may be regarded in either of two ways: in the first place as a mysterious entity, with regard to whose working no law can be definitely predicated, or in the second place, simply as a machine, transforming the energy supplied to it, in ways more or less capable of mechanical explanation. Its move- ments are apparently so diverse that the former of these hypotheses might well seem to be the only alternative. Light, for example, induces sometimes positive curvature, sometimes negative. Gravitation, again, induces one move- ment in the root, and the opposite in the shoot. From these and other reactions it would appear as if the organism had been endowed with various specific sensibilities for its own advantage, and that a consistent mechanical explanation of its movements was therefore out of the question. In spite of this, however, I have attempted to show that the plant may nevertheless be regarded as a machine, and that its movements in response to external stimuli, though apparently ! Paper read before Royal Society, February 4, 1904. PREFACE ix so various, are ultimately reducible to a fundamental unity of reaction. This demonstration has been the object of the present work, and not that treatment of known aspects of plant- movements which is to be found detailed together with the history of the subject, in standard books of reference on plant physiology, such as those of Sachs, Pfeffer, Strasburger, Darwin, Francis Darwin, Vines, and Detmer. In analysing plant-movements the greatest complexity arises from the confusion of effects due to internal energy and external stimulus respectively. I have, however, been able to discriminate the characteristic expressions of these two factors, and thus to disentangle the complex phenomena which result from their combined action. Another very obscure problem is found in the nature of so-called ‘ spon- taneous or autonomous’ movements. By the discovery, however, of multiple response, and by the continuity which I have been able to establish, as existing between multiple and autonomous responses, it has been found possible to demon- strate that there are, strictly speaking, no ‘spontaneous’ movements, those known by this name being really due to external stimulus previously absorbed by the organism. Thus all the experiments have tended to show that the phenomenon of life does not, as such, connote any intrusion into the realm of the organic of a force which would interfere with that law of the Conservation of Energy which is known to hold good in the inorganic world. The elucidation of the fact that such varied and obscure phenomena in the life-processes of the plant, as, for instance, growth and the ascent of sap, are fundamentally due to the same excitatory reactions as are seen otherwise exemplified in the simple mechanical response now familiar to us, con- stituted a further result which, at the outset of the investiga- tion, was little to be foreseen. Xx PLANT ‘RESPONSE It has been shown finally that there is no physiological response given by the most highly organised animal tissue that is not also to be met within the plant. This was proved in detail in the case of the identical polar effects induced in both by electrical currents ; in the conduction of the excitatory impulse to a distance; in the possibility of detecting the excitatory wave in transit and measuring its rate; and in the appropriate modification of its velocity by different agencies, even in the case of ordinary plants ; in the passing of multiple into autonomous response in vegetable tissues ; in the light thrown by this phenomenon on the causes of rhythmicity in animal tissues ; in the similar effects of drugs on animal and vegetable tissues, and in the modifications introduced into these effects by the factor of individual ‘constitution.’ This identity of effects, indeed, as between the responses of plant and animal, is so deep and so extended, that it is to be anticipated that as several of the obscure problems of animal physiology have already been found elucidated by means of these researches carried out on plants, so others will be found capable of explanation by similar means in the near future. In conclusion, I wish to say that from my assistant, Mr. J. Roy, and my pupils, Messrs. A. C. Basu, S. C. Acharya, S. Chakravarty, N. Roy, and S. Goswami, I have received able assistance at various periods during the course of these long and extended investigations. ]. C BORE PRESIDENCY COLLEGE, CALCUTTA: July 1995. CONTENTS PART I—SIMPLE RESPONSE CHAPTER I THE PLANT AS A MACHINE Responsive movements in plants—Work done by plant—Plant as a machine —Indicator-diagrams—Dhysiological response-curves—Pulse-records— Cardiagrams—Modification of pulse by poison and other agencies— Automatic response in plants—Optical Lever Recorder—Effect of external agencies on automatic pulse-beat in plants CHAPTER II MECHANICAL RESPONSE TO STIMULUS Molecular Betaneenent caused by stimulus—Expression in change of form, contraction—Mechanical model—Myograph—Response by differential contraction in pulvinated plant-organs—Longitudinal response in plants —Response of plant to all forms of stimulus—Plant chamber—Practic- able forms of graduated stimuius—Electro-thermic stimulator—Stimula- tion by condenser discharge—Response-recorder—Advantage of counter- poise—Response of Azophytum to thermal stimulation—Response to condenser discharge—-Absolute measurements of motile effect and of work performed—Effect of load—Definite determination of threshold of response—Determination of variation of excitability by measurement of minimally effective stimulus CHAPTER Ill ON THE UNIVERSALITY OF SENSITIVENESS IN PLANTS AS DEMONSTRATED BY MEANS OF ELECTRICAL RESPONSE Arbitrary classification of plants into sensitive and ordinary—Method of r electro-motive variation for detecting state of excitation—Hydraulic model—Excitation of vegetable tissue, like that of animal tissue, induces galvanometric negativity—Methods of direct and transmitted excitation —Electrical and mechanical response alike record molecular derange- ment and recovery—Similarities in simultaneous record of mechanical PAGE 10 Xil PLANT RESPONSE PAGE and electrical response—True excitation has a concomitant negative turgidity-variation, negative mechanical response or fall, and galvano- metric negativity—These are true physiological responses, and are abolished at death—Abnormal positive mechanical and electrical responses brought about by positive turgidity-variation— Direct and indirect effects of stimulation—Discrimination of differences of excit- ability by electric test—Excitability of plant-tissues in general—Re- sponsive power characteristic of matter 2 : : . : Base CHAPTER TV ON CONDITIONS FAVOURABLE TO THE CONSPICUOUS EXHIBITION OF MECHANICAL RESPONSE Differences of degree of motile sensibility in sensitive plants so called— Response of anisotropic organ brought about by differential contraction —Production of response by artificial variation of turgidity—Variation and counter-variation of turgescence, causing two opposite responsive movements— Differences between hydrostatic and true excitatory effects —Distinction of plants as ordinary and sensitive, arbitrary—Sensitive plants may be excited, yet give no mechanical response—Certain con- ditions necessary to exhibition of differential response—Balanced action as result of diffuse stimulus on radial organ—Slight differential contraction of pulvinus magnified by long petiolar index : : : : oa CHAPTERS iV. MECHANICAL RESPONSE IN ORDINARY LEAVES Pulvinoid and pulvinus—Demonstration of mechanical response in ordinary leaves—Response of Artocarpus similar to that of Bzophytum—Response to stimulus, even in old tissues, by expulsion of water—Localisation of motile organ in ordinary leaves—Conducting properties of various tissues —Lamina is not the perceptive organ—Response in ordinary leaves, though sluggish, yet comparable in extent to that of JZ¢mosa —Peculiar phenomenon of fatigue-reversal seen in JZ7zosa observed also in ordinary plants — Periodic reversals . ; : : : ‘ : : «BMS CHAPTER VI LONGITUDINAL RESPONSE OF RADIAL ORGANS Absence of lateral response movements in radial organs due to mutually antagonistic effects of equal contractions of diametrically opposite sides —-Lateral response in radial stem of Walnut under unilateral stimulation --Also in pistil of A/7usa—Diffuse stimulation of radial organ causes longitudinal contraction—The ‘ Kunchangraph ’—Longitudinal contrac- tion of stamens of Cyneree not unique—Similar longitudinal responses CONTENTS obtained with stems, roots, tendrils, petioles, stamens, and styles of ordinary plants—-Also in fungi—Responsive contraction in Pass¢flora, comparable in extent with that in Cyeree— Longitudinal response in plants modified by the physiological variations due to age, season, and chemical agencies CHAPTER VII RESPONSIVE CURVATURE OF MOLECULARLY ANISOTROPIC ORGAN Molecular anisotropy artificially induced by one-sided cooling—Cooled side less responsive --Diffuse stimulation causes concavity of the uncooled, that being relatively the more excitable —Local fatigue diminishes excit- ability—Diffuse stimulation now causes concavity of the unstrained side —Similar anisotropy induced in plagiotropic organs, by unilateral action of light—The lower or shaded side of such organs relatively more excit- able— Diffuse stimulation causes current of response from lower to upper, and also concavity of lower half—Responses of plagiotropic Cucurb7ta and Convolvulus——Differences in excitabilities of outer and inner surfaces of tubular organ— Complex response due to successive excitations of two antagonistic halves of an anisotropic organ—Response of spiral tendrils by uncurling— Response in certain cases by contraction of the spiral or curling —Writhing movement in spiral tendril under strong stimulation . “CHAPTER VIII RELATION BETWEEN STIMULUS AND RESPONSE Ineffective stimulus becomes effective by repetition —Two types of response in contractile animal tissues, cardiac and skeletal— Response of cardiac muscle on ‘all or none’ principle; parallel case in ABiophytem—In skeletal muscle, increasing stimulus causes increasing response, which tends to reach a limit—Parallel results in longitudinal and electrical response of plants—Effect of superposition of stimuli—Tetanus 5 ; 3 PART II.—MODIFICATION OF RESPONSE UNDER VARIOUS CONDITIONS CHAPTER IX ON THE UNIFORM, FATIGUE, AND STAIRCASE EFFECTS IN RESPONSE Uniform response in plants—Staircase effect—Fatigue due to molecular strain—Fatigue in plant-responses—Periodic fatigue—Fatigue under continuous stimulation—Explanation of anomalous erection of leaf of PAGE 66 82 94 xiv PLANT RESPONSE PAGE Mimosa under continuous stimulation—Conductivity and excitability of tissue diminished through incomplete protoplasmic recovery—Relatively greater fatigue in a motile than conducting organ—Disappearance of the motile excitability earlier than conductivity—Refractory period— Absence of responsive effect when stimulus falls within refractory period . /iO3 CHAPTER X THEORIES CONCERNING DIFFERENT TYPES OF RESPONSE The chemical theory of response—Insufficiency of the theory of assimilation and dissimilation to explain fatigue and staircase effects—Similar responsive effects seen in inorganic substances—Molecular theory— When molecular recovery is complete, responses uniform: when incom- plete, fatigue brought about by residual strain—Fatigue under con- tinuous stimulation, in inorganic substance, in plant, and in muscle— Staircase effect brought about by increased molecular mobility : examples seen in inorganic substance, and in living tissues—No sharp line of demarcation in the borderland between physical and chemical pheno- mena—Molecular changes attended by changes of chemical activity— Unequal molecular strain gives rise to a secondary series of chemical actions—Volta-chemical effect and by-products—Supposition that re- sponse always disproportionately larger than stimulus, not justified— Existence of three types: (1) response proportionate to stimulus; (2) response disproportionately greater than stimulus; (3) response dis- proportionately less than stimulus—Instances of stimulus partially held latent: staircase and additive effects; multiple response ; renewed growth : . E . ; : : ; - : ; 2) ARS: CHAPTER XI EFFECT OF ANASTHETICS, POISONS AND OTHER CHEMICAL REAGENTS ON LONGITUDINAL RESPONSE Response modified by physiological change—Carbonic acid causes depression, and transitory exaltation as after-effect—Gradual abolition of response in hydrogen and restoration by access of air—Chemical agents cause con- traction or relaxation of plant-tissue—Effect of alcohol causing temporary exaltation of response followed by depression and protracted period of recovery—FEther causes relaxation and temporary depression of response —Explanation of anomalous action of ether on stimulated J/imodsa leaf —Abolition of response by hydrochloric acid—Response restored by timely application of ammonia—Abolition of response by poisonous reagent — Similarity of effect of chemical agents on the response of animal and vegetable tissues . : : z ; : : : . 1 TZH CONTENTS CHAPTER XII EFFECT OF TEMPERATURE Temperatures optimum, maximum, and minimum—Diminution of electrical response by cooling—Temporary or permanent abolition of response due to cold—Characteristic differences exhibited by different species— Mechanical response of Azophytum and autonomous response of Desmodium arrested by cold—Prolongation of latent period— Diminution of longitudinal mechanical response by cold—Diminution of electrical response of plants by rise of temperature—Similar diminution seen in longitudinal mechanical response—Increase of excitability due to cyclic variation of temperature. : . : : CHAPTER XIII ON THE DEATH-SPASM IN PLANTS . Difficulty of determining exact moment of death—Various Jost-mortem symptoms afford no immediate indication—Ideal methods for determina- tion of death-point—Realised in four different ways: (a) Determination by electrical method—(4) Determination by spasmodic lateral movement at moment of death—Experiments with 4/zosa—Death-contraction a true physiological response—Continuity of fatigue and death—Death- point earlier in young tissues—Composite spasmodic movement—(é’) Determination of death-point in tendril of Pass¢fora, by sudden move- ment of uncurling—(c) Determination of death-point by method of volu- metric contraction of hollow organ, causing expulsion of contained water CHAPTER: XIV THE DETERMINATION OF THE CRITICAL POINT OF DEATH BY INVERSION OF THE THERMO-MECHANICAL CURVE J j Death-spasm in anisotropic organ due to differential longitudinal contraction . —In radial organ the death-contraction is purely longitudinal—Death- : point determined from point of inversion of a thermo-mechanical curve ; —The complete record thus constitutes a curve of life-and-death, the two being separated by the death-point—Characteristic thermo-mechanical | curve as resultant of variation of temperature and variation of length— The necessity of specifying the rate of rise of temperature—The thermo- mechanical curve characterised by sharp and definite inversion at point of death—No inversion of thermo-mechanical curve after death of plant ——Death-contraction under heat-rigor in plant analogous to similar phenomenon in animal—The J/orograph, a perfected form of apparatus for determining critical point of death—Remarkable identity of thermo- mechanical curves obtained with two similar specimens—Death-point almost as definite as a physical constant—Vanishing of point of inversion with age—Determination of death-point under cold-rigor—Constancy of death- point : : ; : - XV PAGE 159 XVI PLANT RESPONSE CHAPTER XV EFFECT OF VARIOUS AGENCIES ON DEATH-RESPONSE: THERMOGRAPHS OF REGIONAL DEATH Lowering of death-point by fatigue —Modification of characteristic thermo- mechanical curve by the action of chemical agents-—Comparison- Morograph—Duplication of rigor-point—Death-response a physiological response and not due to coagulation—Death-movements of flowers— Approximate constancy of death-point of florets in a capitulum—Definite interval between death-point and discoloration-point—Translocation of discoloration-point by various agencies—Thermographs of regional death—Thermograph of local fatigue—Thermographic investigation of electrotonic excitation PART III—EXCITABILITY AND CONDUCTIVIG® CHAPTER XVI ON EXCITATORY POLAR EFFECTS OF CURRENTS Hydro-mechanical theory of excitation in plants—Theory of protoplasmic change—Crucial tests applied by means of polar excitation—Mono-polar and Bi-polar methods of excitation—Advantages of study of polar excitation in plant-tissues as compared with animal—Effects of feeble E.M.F.—Effect of moderately high E.M.F.—Experiments with on excitable tissues . : 3 - - - : - CHAPTER XVII ON CONDITIONS OF REVERSAL OF NORMAL POLAR EFFECTS IN LIVING TISSUES Effect of high E.M.F.—Effects at two stages, 4 and A—Experimental verification of 4 stage effect—Similar effects seen in protozoa—Experi- mental verification of complete reversal at B stage —Law of polar effects under high E.M.F.—Investigation on polar effects by death-response— Reversal of polar effects as due to fatigue, or tissue-modification—In- vestigation of polar effects by glow-response of fireflies . : : C CHAPTER XVIII ON CONDUCTIVITY AND EXCITABILITY Receptive excitability, conductivity, and motile excitability—Molecular Model— Modification of motile excitability : (a) by anzesthetics—(6) by PAGE 176 200 CONTENTS XVii PAGE cold—(c) by fatigue—Variation of conductivity: (a2) by cold—(4) by rise of temperature—(c) by fatigue—(d@) by anesthetics—Variation of receptive excitability by ether—Conductivity versus excitability — Abolition of motile excitability without abolition of conductivity— Hydro-mechanical theory of transmission of stimulus untenable. = 216 CHAPTER XIX ON ELECTROTONUS The anode acts as a block to the transmission of stimulus—Opposite effect of kathode—Experiments on Azophytum, showing variations of con- ductivity by anode and kathode respectively Experiments on J/¢mosa, showing increase of motile excitability at or near the kathode, and diminution of motile excitability at or near the anode-- Curious ‘ develop- ment’ of response, near the kathode . : : . 3 ‘ Sergi CHAPTER XX ON THE VELOCITY OF TRANSMISSION OF EXCITATORY WAVES IN PLANTS Difficulties in accurate determination of velocity of transmission, due to unknown variations of excitability arising from injury, and variations of conductivity through fatigue—A perfect method of obtaining accurate and consistent results—Relative advantages of studying conduction in plants as compared with animals—Determinations of velocity of trans- mission in centripetal and centrifugal directions — Preferential conductivity in centrifugal direction—Diminution of conductivity and excitability by fatigue—Within a certain critical interval, organ ‘refractory’ to further stimulus—Increased velocity of transmission with increasing stimulus— Measurement of diminution of conductivity by cold—Fibro-vascular elements the best conducting channels—Conductivity lengthwise greater than crosswise—Electric mode of determination of velocity of trans- mission—Indifferent parenchymatous tissues practically not transmitters of excitation—Comparative tables showing velocity of transmission in various plant and animal tissues . ‘ - ; : NO 1os) eo CHAPTER XXI ON DETECTION OF EXCITATORY PULSE DURING TRANSIT BY ELECTROTACTILE AND ELECTROMOTIVE METHODS Pfeffer’s experiment on expulsion of water from excited cells—Author’s experiment on a delicate method of detecting excitatory expulsion of cell- sap—Chemical method of determining velocity of transmission of excita- tion—Electrotactile detector—Demonstration of passage of excitatory a XViii PLANT RESPONSE contractile wave by means of electrotactile method—Determination of velocity of transmission of excitation in ordinary plants by electromotive method—Excitatory verses hydro-mechanical movement of water . CHAPTER XXII THE LATENT PERIOD AND REFRACTORY PERIOD The determination of the latent period in 4/¢osa—FExperimental arrange- ments for obtaining automatic record—Prolongation of latent period by cold—Spark-record for determination of latent period—Prolongation of latent period by fatigue—Sluggishness of the response of Phd/anthus urinaria, also long latent period and very protracted period of recovery —Latent period reduced under strong stimulation—Response in Biophytum on the ‘all or none’ principle—Definite value of effective stimulus—Phenomenon of refractory period in Bzophytim—Parallelism of responses in Azophytum and in cardiac muscle—Additive effects— Inappropriateness of term ‘refractory period ’—Energy in excess of effective stimulus held latent for subsequent manifestation PART IV.—MULTIPLE AND AUTONOMOUS RESPONSE CHAPTER XXIII ON MULTIPLE RESPONSE Multiple electromotive responses due to a single strong stimulus—Multiple electrotactile responses—-Multiple mg¢chanical responses in Bzophytum— Cyclic variations in multiple responses—Multiple retinal excitations— Intermittent pulse in man and plant—Semi-automatism—Continuity of multiple and automatic response —Conversion of Azophytum into auto- matically responding plant ; conversion of Desmodium into ordinarily responding plant—Similar polar effects of current in Azophyéum and in Desmodium \eaflet at standstill—Moderate stimulus in Azophytum and in Desmodium at standstill produces single response ; and strong stimulus, multiple response ; CHAPTER XXIV PAGE 254 264 279 AN INQUIRY INTO THE CAUSES OF AUTONOMOUS MOVEMENTS Production of pulsatory movements as after-effect of energy absorbed— Physical analogue — Localisation of seat of automatic excitation in Des- modium— Demonstration of multiple response to a constant stimulus : (1) Chemical—(2) Electrical—(3) Stimulus of light—Multiple response CONTENTS to constant stimulus of light, in: (a) retina—(4) Bzophytum—(c) Des- modium—(4) Thermal—Induction of automatism in Azophytum at favourable temperature—(5) Of internal hydrostatic pressure—Absorp- tion of external energy and its absorption by the plant in latent form— True meaning of ‘ tonic’ condition—Cause of rhythmicity—After-effect, and its relative persistence . CHAPTER XXV INFLUENCE OF VARIOUS CHEMICAL REAGENTS ON THE AUTONOMOUS RESPONSE OF DESMODIUM GYRANS The recorder and experimental chamber—Absolute measurement of period and amplitude of Desmodzem-oscillation—lKesponsive significance of up and down movements deduced from (a) analogy with response of Mimosa ; (6) test of increased internal hydrostatic pressure—‘ Systolic’ contraction and ‘ diastolic’ expansion of Desmodium pulvinus—Mode of application of chemical reagents—Action of chemical reagents modified by: tonic condition of plants; strength of solution; and duration of application—Effect of anzesthetics—Effect of alcohol— Effect of carbonic acid—Effects of ammonia and of carbon disulphide—Effect of copper sulphate solution, either when applied externally, direct on the pulvinus, or internally—Spark-record of Desmodium-pulsation CHAPTER oc XVI EFFECTS OF TEMPERATURE ON AUTONOMOUS RESPONSES Increase of frequency and diminution of amplitude of pulsation with rising temperature —Converse effect of fall of temperature — Similar effect in cardiac pulsation—Effect of the reduction of temperature to the thermo- tonic minimum—Explanation of diminution of amplitude of pulsation with rise of temperature—Anomalous use of the word ‘ relaxation ’— Simple verses additive character of individual pulsation CHAPTER XXVII SIMILARITIES OF RHYTHMIC RESPONSE IN VEGETABLE ANI ANIMAL TISSUES ~ The similarities, in their fundamental characteristics, of rhythmic tissues, animal and vegetable: (1) In responses—(2) In possession of long re- fractory periods—(3) In incapability of tetanus—Theories regarding the causation of heart-beat—The similarities of rhythmic tissues, animal and vegetable, as seen in: (1) The effects of internal hydrostatic pressure— (2) The effects of variation of temperature—(3) The periodic groupings a2 PAGE 295 SE XX PLANT RESPONSE of response—(4) The effect of barium salt—(5) The antagonistic actions of acid and alkali—Identity of rhythmic phenomena in animal and vegetable tissues PART V.—ASCENT OF SAP CHAPTER XXVIII SUCTIONAL RESPONSE AND ASCENT OF SAP Inadequacy of existing theories of ascent of sap—General considerations regarding cellular activity and resultant propulsion of water—The Shoshungraph—Balanced Shoshungraph for determining variations of suction—Hydrostatic and Hydraulic Methods of Balance CHAPTER XXIX MODIFICATION OF SUCTIONAL RESPONSE . Effect of temperature on suction by three methods of inquiry: (1) Un- balanced method of Shoshungraph: (a2) Action of cold-—(d) Action of moderate rise of temperature—(2) Method of Hydrostatic Balance : (a) Action of cold—Reversal of normal direction of flow—(é) Action of warm water—(3) Method of Hydraulic Balance—(a) Action of cold— (6) Effect of warm water—-Explanation of suction when the root is killed by boiling water—Stimulation renews suctional activity in plant whose suction has come to a standstill—Osmotic versus excitatory action —Abolition of suction by poison—Suctional activity continued until whole plant is killed by poison CHAPTER XXX THE PHENOMENON OF PROPULSION OF SAP AND ITS VARIOUS EFFECTS The mechanics of the ascent of sap: (a) Uni-directioned flow—(¢) Initiation of multiple rhythmic excitations—Connection between conduction of excitation and conduction of sap—Rapidity of ascent of sap accounted for by stimulatory action—Positive and negative pressures due to one cause—(1) Positive pressure—-(2) Negative pressure—(3) Irregular variations of pressure—Direct conduction and conduction by relays— Excretion of water—Excretion of nectar—Translocation of organic food- substances—Mechanical response to suctional activity—-Effect of warmth —Effect of cold—Explanation of the drooping of leaves during frost— Explanation of response and recovery—Antagonistic actions of internal energy and external stimulus PAGE 344 359 372 389 Me: CONTENTS XX1 PART VI.—GROWTH CHAPTER XXXI THE RECORD OF GROWTH-RESPONSE PAGE The simple Crescograph—The Balanced Crescograph—Rhythmic growth- response—Growth-response and excitatory response—Law of direct and indirect effects of excitation— Positive turgidity-variation as indirect effect of excitation—Mechanical test—-Significance of ‘inner stimuli’. - 409 CHAPTER XXXII THE EFFECTS ON GROWTH OF INTERNAL ENERGY AND EXTERNAL STIMULUS Characteristics common to growth and to other forms of rhythmic response : (1) Periodic groupings—(2) Effect of external stimulus in renewal of growth when at temporary standstill—(3) Renewal of growth-pulsation by positive turgidity-variation—(4) Effect of increased internal hydro- static pressure—(5) Effect of ascent of sap on growth—Effect of tempera- ture on growth—Comparison of various types of multiple response— Effect of external tension on growth—Effect of direct application of stimulus on the growing region—Similarities between motile and growth responses—Direct and indirect effects of stimulus, and laws of growth . 424 CHAPTER XXXIII ON THE RELATION BETWEEN TEMPERATURE AND GROWTH, AND THE ACCURATE DETERMINATION OF OPTIMUM AND MAXIMUM POINTS General consideration of difficulties of accurate determination of effects of temperature on growth—Four accurate methods : (1) Method of discon- tinuous observations — Accurate regulation of temperature by electrolytic rheostat—(2) Method of continuous observations—Thermo-crescent curve—Determination of the optimum point—(3) Method of balance— (4) Method of excitatory response—Translocation of the optimum point 441 CHAPTER XXXIV ON AN ATTEMPT TO DETECT AND MEASURE LATENT STIMULUS, AND ON THE STUDY OF PERIODIC AFTER-EFFECTS Positive and negative after-effects—Extreme delicacy of the Method of Balance—Detection of absorbed stimulus by negative after-effect— XXii PLANT RESPONSE PAGE Constancy of sum of direct and indirect after-effects—Latent component almost vanishing above the optimum—Variation of receptivity—Direct and indirect response of plant in sub-tonic condition—Table showing direct and indirect effects at different temperatures—Is the change induced by stimulus always of an explosive chemical character ?—Rela- tion between stimulus and response in different tonic conditions—After- effect—Factors which determine periodic after-effects: (1) Stimulus of light—(2) Temperature—(3) Chemical stimulus—(4) Turgidity—Con- tinuous photographic record of the pulsations of Desvzodium—Record of periodic variation of rate of growth—Continuous photographic record of periodic variations of transpiration—Continuous photographic record of the variation of the rate of growth——Annual rings and seasonal periodicity - 456 CHAPTER XXXV AN INVESTIGATION INTO THE DIFFERENT EFFECTS OF DRUGS ON PLANTS OF DIFFERENT * CONSTITUTIONS’ General consideration of the problem—‘ Constitution’ and the elements which determine it—Methods of investigation—Action of carbonic acid gas—Action of ether—Effect of solution of sodium carbonate—Effect of solution of sugar—Effect of alcohol—Effect of acids—Effect of alkali— Antagonistic action of alkalis and acids—Action of strong solution of sodium chloride—Effect of poisonous solution of copper sulphate — Opposite effects of the same dose on different constitutions —Opposite effects of large and small doses. ; : : : : - 5 Age PART VII.—GEOTROPISM, CHEMOTROPISM, AND GALVANOTROPISM CHAPTER XXXVI THE RESPONSIVE CURVATURES CAUSED BY GRAVITY. NEGATIVE GEOTROPISM Statement of the problem of apogeotropic response— Mode in which stimu- lation is brought about : radial-pressure theory, and theory of statoliths —Mechanics of responsive movement—Experiment demonstrating re- sponsive curvature as brought about by unilateral pressure of particles— Record of curvature induced by gravitation—KRecord of different rates of curvature when specimen is held at angles of 45° and 135° to the vertical—Determination of the true character of apogeotropic response —Responsive curvature of acellular organs—Curvature of grass haulm under gravity—Growth of grass haulm on a klinostat . : : ©2493 CONTENTS XXill CHAPTER XXXVII THE RESPONSIVE PECULIARITIES OF THE TIPS OF : GROWING ORGANS PAGE Difference between shoot and root in their response to stimulus of gravity —Difference in character of response between tip and growing region of root—Scope of the investigation—Electrical investigation— Responsive results of: 1. Longitudinal transmission of effect of stimulus from tip ; (az) Moderate unilateral stimulation ; (4) effect of stronger unilateral stimulation—2. Direct unilateral stimulation of growing region— Moderately strong stimulus—3. Transverse transmission of effect of stimulus; (a) moderate stimulation; (4) stronger stimulation— Mechanical response inferred from observed electrical response— Tabular statement . : : é c : d ‘ . 5 SSe CHAPTER XXXVIII INQUIRY INTO THE LAWS OF RESPONSIVE GROWTH- CURVATURES Scope of the investigation: 1. Mechanical response to unilateral stimula- tion of the tips of shoot and root: (a) Moderate stimulus—(é) Stronger stimulation—z2. Effect of unilateral stimulus, applied at the responding growing region: (a) Moderate stimulus—(é) Strong or long-continued stimulus—Experiments on the direct and indirect effects of stimulus on Mimosa: (a) Direct stimulation—(4) Indirect stimulation, longitudinal transmission—(c) Indirect stimulation, transverse transmission—The curious response of 47/scma—Table showing responsive effects common to pulvini, pulvinoids, and growing organs—Laws of responsive growth- curvature . . . : . : : . : 5 : ee 5 2z: CHAPTER. XXXIX INQUIRY INTO POSITIVE GEOTROPISM No specific difference as regards their responses, between shoot and root— Darwinian curvature—-Localisation of geotropic sensibility at the root- tip—Experiments as to whether amputation of root-tip abolishes excitability—The tip of the root the organ of gravi-perception—The perceptive versus the responding organ—True perceptive region . . 536 CHAPTER XL ON CHEMO-TROPISM AND GALVANO-TROPISM General difficulties of the investigation— How to overcome these difficulties— Three distinct methods of testing results: (1) by variation of longitudinal XXiV PLANT RESPONSE PAGE growth ; (2) by responsive movement of pulvinus; and (3) by growth- curvature— Method of application of chemical reagent—Effect of alkali —Effect of acid—Effect of copper sulphate—Action of sugar solution— Chemo-tactic movements—Explanation of anomalous osmotic or plas- molytic action—Excitatory verses plasmolytic reaction in pulvinus of Mimosa: (1) Favourable tonic condition—(2) Ordinary tonic condition -—Polar effects of currents inducing growth-curvatures—Localised polar effects on pulvinus—Anodic and kathodic effects on longitudinal growth —Generalised law of polar excitation in plants—Galvano-tropic response —The indirect effect of polar excitation—The effect on growth of ‘ electrification ’ of soil ; , : : ; ; : : == ehAO PART VIII.—HELIOTROPISM ; CHAPTER XLI FUNDAMENTAL RESPONSIVE ACTION OF PLANT-TISSUES TO STIMULUS OF LIGHT Diversity of movements induced by light—Differentiation of responsive movements—Action of light on tissues in sub-tonic condition—Effect of light on pulvinated organs—Effect of diffuse stimulation of light on non-growing radial organs—Retarding effect of light on longitudinal growth—Phenomenon of oscillation under long-continued stimulation— Similarity of responsive reaction under light and under other forms of stimulation ‘ . ; : : ; ; F , ; « = 565 CHAPTER XLII POSITIVE HELIOTROPISM Introduction—Theory of de Candolle—Inadequacy of de Candolle’s theory —Definition of terms positive and negative—Darwin’s theory of modified circumnutation—Response of terminal leaflet of Desmodium— Extreme sensitiveness of some plant-organs to light—Merging of multiple in continuous response—Orientation induced by light—The perceptive region in the terminal leaflet of Dessodi’um—Heliotropic response in radial organ—Magnetically controlled recorder—Heliotropic response of hypocotyl of Szzapzs—Recovery and theory of recti-petality. 579 CHAPTER XLIII NEGATIVE HELIOTROPISM Incomplete parallelism between actions of light and of gravitation— Theoretical considerations—Kecording microscope—Negative _helio- CONTENTS XXV PAGE tropic curvature induced by stimulation of the tips of root and shoot— Intermediate phases between positive and negative heliotropic response : (a) neutralisation by transverse transmission; (4) neutralisation by transverse transmission, with multiple response—Localised sensitiveness to light and transmission of excitatory effect—Negative heliotropism of a radial organ—Gradual transition from positive to negative, through intermediate phase of neutrality—Apparent heliotropic insensitiveness of certain tendrils—Negative heliotropismn of tendril of Vitzs . : = Soy CHAPTER XLIV EFFECT OF INVISIBLE RADIATION AND EMANATIONS Effect of temperature and its variations—Demonstration of fundamental effect of thermal radiation on growth—Kesponse to successive uniform stimuli of thermal radiation—FEffect of continuous unilateral stimulation —Effect of electrical waves on growth— Response of A/zmosa to electric radiation—Action of high frequency Tesla current : ; : - 614 CHAPTER XLV ON PHOTONASTIC PHENOMENA AND ON DIURNAL SLEEP Photonasty and para-heliotropism—Response of 77ope@olum majus—Re- sponses of plagiotropic stems : (a) J/?mosa—(b) [pomaa—(c) Cucurbita— Daily periodic movements of plagiotropic stems— Responsive movements of pulvinated organs—Pulvinated organs showing positive heliotropic movement: (z) Response of terminal leaflet of Dessodzum—(b) Response of leaflet of Rodzz7a —(c) Responsive movements of leaflets of Axythrina indica and Clitoria ternatea—The negative heliotropic type of response : (a) Response of pulvinus of AZz70sa—(6) Diurnal sleep of Oxalzs—(c) Diurnal sleep of Bzophytum-—Directive verses non-directive action of light—General view of responsive curvatures induced in different organs by unilateral application of light . : : ; ; : > 1 O02 CHAPTER XLVI ON DIA-HELIOTROPISM AND DIA-GEOTROPISM Difficulty of distinguishing between effect of light and other reactions— Theories of Frank and De Vries—Subsidiary factors: (1) Epinasty and hyponasty ; (2) Effect of gravity; (3) Effect of suctional activity and of turgescence; (4) Modification of effect by characteristic limits of flexibility—Discrimination of the part played by heliotropism in the movement of the leaf—Proof of absence of any specific dia-heliotropic tendency in leaves—The lamina not the perceptive organ— Principal types of the response of leaves to stimulus of light—Positive type of response ;: mango leaf—Negative type of response: leaf of Artocarpus . 640 XXV1 PLANT RESPONSE CHAPTER XLVII TORSIONAL RESPONSE TO HELIOTROPIC AND GEOTROPIC STI- MULUS: AUTONOMOUS TORSION AND ITS VARIATIONS PAGE Torsional effect—Method of recording torsional response—Torsional re- sponse under the lateral action of light-—-Torsional response to other forms of lateral stimulation—Torsional response of compound strip of ebonite and stretched india-rubber—Modification of torsional response by artificial variation of the relative excitabilities of the two halves— Laws of torsional response—Demonstration of differential geotropic ex- citation in a dorsi-ventral organ—Torsional response to lateral geotropic stimulation—Modification of torsional geotropic response by artificial variation of differential excitability—Autonomous torsion: Effect of temperature—Effect of light—Effect of electrical current—Effect of gravity—The twining of stems . : : ; : ‘ : . 656 CHAPTER XLVIII NYCTITROPIC MOVEMENTS Comparison between nyctitropic and autonomous pulsations—Diurnal move- ment of plagiotropic stem—Supposed distinction between nyctitropic and other movements of response to stimulus of light—Diurnal response of leaf of Bzophytum—Diurnal response of primary petiole of AZmosa— Periodic impulses acting on the leaf—Periodic impulses contributed by the plant as a whole—Other modes of exhibition of diurnal periodicity of hydrostatic tension—Forced vibration and its periodic after-effects— Physical analogue—Impressed periodic vibrations in organ originally radial , : : ; : : : . F : : --, 070 CHAPTER XLIX ON PULSATORY RESPONSE AND SWIMMING MOVEMENTS AS INITIATED AND MODIFIED BY LIGHT AND OTHER FORMS OF STIMULUS Investigations on the influence of light on the lateral leaflet of Desmodium gyrans: (a) in sub-tonic condition; (4) in normal tonic condition— Changes induced in existing anisotropy of Desmodium leaflets—Reversal under intense stimulation seen in all forms of response—The swimming movements of ciliated organisms—Fundamental resemblance between the swimming responses and the ordinary heliotropic responses in radial organs—Phototactic movements: (2) Two natural types of responsive movements; (4) Responsive movement positive, negative, or inter- mediate, according to intensity of stimulation—Directive action of light —Thermotaxis—Galvanotaxis—Chemotaxis ; ; : : . 689 CONTENTS XXVli PART IX.—GENERAL SURVEY AND CONCEUSION CHAPTER L REVIEW OF RESPONSE, SIMPLE AND MULTIPLE PAGE Responsive contraction—Kunchangraphic records—Direct and_ indirect effects of stimulation—Various forms of responsive expression; (a) Lateral motile response by differential contraction ; (4) Suctional re- sponse ; (c) Growth-response; (d@) Torsional response; (e) Death response ; (/) Thermographs of regional death ; (g) Electrical response —Different types of response: (a) Uniform response; (4) Fatigue ; (c) Staircase response—Excitability—Conductivity—Polar effects of currents—Multiple response—Continuity of multiple and autonomous responses—The ascent of sap. : ¢ 5 : > j E708 CHAPTER LI RESPONSIVE GROWTH-CURVATURES IN PLANTS Longitudinal growth and its variations—Effect of temperature on growth— Responsive growth-curvature under unilateral stimulation :—1. Direct unilateral stimulus on the responding organ: (a) Positive response under moderate stimulation ; (4) Intermediate or neutral response ; (c) Negative response ; (¢) Dorsi-ventral positive response ; (e) Dorsi-ventral response which may become negative—2. Indirect effect of uni- lateral stimulation: (a) Negative response; (4) Positive response— Responsive action under stimulus of gravity—Heliotropic action in radial organs—Heliotropic action in plagiotropic and dorsi-ventral organs— Phototactic movements—-Nyctitropic movements . : : - m2 CHAPTER LIT ON PHYSIOLOGICAL RESPONSE, AND ITS CONTINUITY IN PLANT AND ANIMAL Vitalism —Fundamental unity of physiological response in plant and animal —Theory of Darwin—Variation as induced by external forces ; 40 CLASSIFIED LIST OF EXPERIMENTS é : : - mae 8 NE tar Seta eg aes FIG. AY EOP ILLUSTRATIONS Record of Healthy Adult and Senile Human Pulse (Broadbent) Effect of Muscarin in arresting Pulsation of Frog’s Ventricle (Cushing) 4 Record of Human Pulse (Broadbent) ' z 3 : : : 4 Demonstration Optical Pulse- Recorder 7 Death of Plant, and Arrest of Pulsation, by Poison 8 Death, and Arrest of Pulsation, in Leaflet of Desmodium by Serie Electric Shock : : é ; : - ; - alts 8 Response of India-rubber 3 A : : : ‘ : pe tA Mechanical Lever Recorder ; : : : 3 So ee me Series of Contractile Responses in Mase . : - Leas Photographic Record of Longitudinal oemeenie Response in ordinary Stamens (Brownea ariza) . ; : eg MOS Differential Lateral Response of Compound Sree ‘ ; : : 3 Plant Chamber and Recorder ‘ é : ; : ; me 16 The Electro-thermic Stimulator : : 17 Diagram of Connections for Stimulation ee Cordes Dicharee : 18 Photographic Record of Response and Recovery of ae taken on a slowly moving drum . : : 21 Photographic Record of Rene i in difisrent specimen, plea on a faster-moving drum ; : : S er Ol Response of Bzophyten to Thermal en ra , : . oe Response of Szophytum to Electric Stimulation . : : Wiser 2 Effect of Load : , : ; : : : oe 25 The Spiral Spring- Peed: . : 2 - . : Se 445) Isometric Response of J/zmosa : . : Sy eee Hydraulic Model for Explanation of Blectrie Ronee : Sit ae Electrical Response in Plant by Method of Block 3 Electric Response Recorder . : : : : re Se Method of Transmitted Stimulation . é i= 3 Simultaneous Mechanical and Electrical ee pasces in 1 Beppe - 3 The Abnormal Positive preceding the Normal Negative in Mechanical and Electrical Responses in 4zophytum : : : : 55 BY) Response of Selenium to the Stimulus of Light. : ; ae -30 XXX PLANT RESPONSE FIG. 29. Response of Metal abolished by the action of ‘ Poison’ (Oxalic Acid). 30. Responses of quickly reacting Azophytum, and sluggish Philanthus urtnaria, wider moderate and under stronger Stimulation 31. Artificial Hydraulic Response of J/zmosa 2. Response of Ordinary Leaf (Artocarpus) . : 33. Response of Leaves of Ordinary Plants to Electric Samus 34. Alternate opposite-directioned Responses obtained by the successive Unilateral Stimulations of opposite sides of Pistil of AZusa 35. The Kunchangraph 36. Diagrammatic ares a of eters for Perieidis Summiane of Plant 37. Response of Stem of Cuscauta to emienae Saowienssd 38. Photographic Record of Responses of Style of Datura alba to Thermal Stimulation ; 39. Responses of Piesnecrie Stem of Cue uw) ie 40. Responses of Plagiotropic Stem of Convoluilus , 41. Response of Bifurcated 4//im Tube by sudden Collapse 42. Responses of Hooked Tendril of Passzflora ~ 43. Response by Coiling of spirally-cut 4//iem Peaaecte 44. Ineffective Stimulus made Effective by Repetition 45. Additive Effect in Electrical Response . rete 46. Mechanical Responses to Stimuli increasing in Arthnchel Propeeeion 47. Curve showing Relation between Stimulus and Response 48. Increased Electrical Response with Increasing Vibrational Simul . (Cauliflower-stalk) 49. Genesis of Tetanus in Muscle 50. Photographic Record of Genesis of Tetanus in a MecHanieall Réspalnee a Plants (Style of Datura alba) 51. Uniform Electrical Responses (Radish) : 52. Staircase Effect in Longitudinal Mechanical Response af Plant (Style of Zucharis) 53. Fatigue in Teyieetal Mechapil ope ri Plant (Style , Datura). 5 54.. Fatigue shown in Bieccal Resmmee) a gugicae Tine is ao allowed for Full Recovery 55. Alternate Fatigue in Electrical Responses of Peucle of Cantona : in Multiple Electric Responses of Peduncle of Azophytum ; in Multiple Mechanical Responses of Leaflet of Azophytum ; and in Autonomous Responses of Desmodium ; 56. Rapid Fatigue under Continuous Stimulation in Masala, ane in Leafstalk of Celery . - , 57. Fatigue under long-continued Simuletion: in the Gonersete Reauanee of Plants 58. Photographic Record a Penodic Fatigue onde Contintans Sttannlas tion in Contractile Response (Filament of Urzc/és Lily) . Fi 59. Photographic Records of Normal Response of J/imosa to Single Stimulus (upper ses and to Continuous Stimulation (lower figure) 108 109 ILLUSTRATIONS XXX PAGE Ineffectiveness of Stimuli, owing to Increasing Fatigue, in A/zmosa 113 Fatigue-Reversal in Arsenic, under Continuous Stimulation of Hertzian Radiation : 119 Automatic Record of Togs in the Gaaieicale. Respasde oF Tada rubber under Rapidly Succeeding Thermal Shocks 120 Preliminary Staircase Increase, followed by Fatigue, in the Beene of Galena to Hertzian Radiation 121 Preliminary Staircase Increase, followed by je Paves in ihe Regione: of Style of Zucharis 7 121 Preliminary Staircase, followed by Patience, in Te: ponies sf Muscle (Brodie) 122 Staircase Increase in Electrical esate an Petiole ae Bi eee. rendered sluggish by cooling 122 Mechanical Response in Tendril of Pan oiere in tien Growl was originally at Standstill 127 Effect of Carbonic Acid Gas on an audtacel Goiceenie Repohee 131 Effect of Hydrogen Gas . 132 Photographic Record showing Effect of Caifiont Teer ateee in Abgkish- ing Response : . 5 . : : 132 Effect of Vapour of Alcohol 134 Effect of Ether : 135 Effect of HCl Vapour 136 Action of Chlorine . 137 Diminution of Response in Zc bate zs Lily he amas of Temiperatare 140 After-effect of Cold on Ivy, Holly, and Eucharis 141 Effect of Cold on Longitudinal Response 143 Effect of Rise of Temperature on Electrical Beene 144 Effect of Rise of Temperatureo n ae, Sis Contractile ees of Plant. 144 Effect of Rising and Falling Memperitare on the ‘Piceaiel Response of Scotch Kale (stimulus constant) 145 Effect of Cyclic Rise and Fall of cea ceetinte on onpianiane) Mechanical Response in Plant 146 Determination of Death-point in A/dzum Tube by Gheeeeaiinn af Volumetric Contraction, causing sudden Expulsion of Water . 157 The Thermometric Spiral and Optic Lever of the Morograph 165 The Morograph 167 Thermo-mechanical Curve eueincd! Phatoeraphieally (Geil Fila. ment of Passiflora) . 168 Thermo-mechanical Curve of Two Digercat Suerte af Sai of Datura alba, obtained from Flowers of the same Plant . 169 Thermo-mechanical Records of Young Specimen of Sfzvrogyra ; Older Specimen of same ; and Style of Datura alba . 170 Thermo-mechanical Record of Leaf of AZzmosa 172 Diagrammatic Representation of Mono-polar Excitation 193 Bi-polar Excitation of Mimosa 194 Bi-polar Excitation of Bzophytum 194 Make-kathode and Break-anode Effects in Hiapigian XXNXil PLANT RESPONSE FIG. 93. Record of Responses of Leaflet of Azophytum, showing Responses occurring at Kathode at Make and not at Break ; and at Anode at Break and not at Make 94. Effects of Ascending and Descending (ened on mee Excitable Specimen of AZz0sa : 95. Molecular Model Exhibiting Beciability at He Revsatine eer Conductivity of Intervening Region; and Mechanical Response of Terminal Responder : A218 96. Effect of Ether in the Abolition of Motile Excieabilay: - - : 97. Experimental Demonstration of Effects of Cold and Anzsthetics in Abolishing Conductivity . : : 98. Diagrammatic Representation of ecient: on Biophyhame 99. Effect of Anode as Block . : ‘ : 100. Experiment showing the Transmission of Byciatowy Wave! through Kathodic Area, and its Stoppage by the Anode. : - ror. Demonstration of Simultaneous Opposite Effects of Anode aaa Kathode on Transmission of Excitation : 102. Diagrammatic Representation of Electrical Conedeiaa in Midas to Exhibit Variation of Motile Excitability, induced by Anode and Kathode : : : : ; : : 103. ‘ Developing’ Action of Kaihods ; 104. Diagrammatic Representation of Electrical Consectine for ‘Detect nation of Velocities of Centrifugal and Centripetal Transmissions . 105. Curve showing Decline in Heights of Responses, with Diminishing Periods of Rest : 106. Electrotactile Method for Datedlion of Recueer wave aainiel Transit 107. Electrotactile Renee in Stem of ade 108. Experimental Arrangements for Simultaneous Recoiae of Mecans cal and Electrical Responses 109. Apparatus for Automatic Record , 110. Photographic Record of Response of JZ¢mosa, Exhibiting tiie Laven Period in its Variation. 111. Electric Spark Record, showing faces a Latent Benga ty Fatigue, in Successive Responses of a Leaf of AZmosa 5 112. Response of Azophytum; Electrical Stimulus ae bead Applied at the Pulvinus.of the Motile Leaflet 113. Additive Effects seen in Responses of Binal to Stim wtiat Fall outside the Refractory Period : 114. Multiple Electrical Responses due to Single Beane Sieanie i : 11§. Multiple Electrotactile Response in Stem of J/imosa, due to Single Strong Thermal Stimulus 116. Multiple Mechanical Response of inapiston age to a ‘Siaule Thermal Stimulus : . . ‘ . . 117. Multiple Response in Bzophytum : : . : : * 118. Multiple Response in Biophytum . : 119. Multiple Response in Eee showin Cyelic Grouping af Amplitude and Period. é . PAGE 196 197 218 220 222 224 233 233 234 235 236 242 246 259 . 260 261 266 268 269 270 274 280 280 283 285 285 286 FIG. 120. 721. 122. 123. 124. 125. 126. 127. 128. 129. 130. neh ie 132. 533. 134. 135. 136. Bz}. 138. 139. 140. 141. 142. 143. 144. 145. 146. 147. 148. 149. 150. ILLUSTRATIONS XXXili Multiple Response in Azophytum, showing ee" eae Intermittent Human Pulse (Broadbent) Intermittence in Pulsation of Azophytum . Multiple Response of Bzophytum under the aeadens meaen of Light c Induction of Ba tonouione Respanee: in Bictlipnand: at Moderately High Temperature of 35° C. : Experimental Apparatus for Making Records af Bulseen of Desieo- dium... 2 Photographic Record of palauan of ee Photographic Record of Uniform Pulsations in Desmodium . Displacement of Mean Position of Vibration of Desmodium Lenilet by Increased Internal Hydrostatic Pressure . : Method of Application of Chemical. Agent to Cut End af Petiole Photographic Record of Effect of Ether Vapour, Large Dose Photographic Record of Effect of Alcohol . : Photographic Record of Effect of Carbonic Acid Gas Photographic Record of Effect of Copper Sulphate Solution Applied on the Pulvinus Spark-record of Single Bateson in Leader of Demaione : Photographic Records of Autonomous Pulsations in Desmodium, showing Increase of Amplitude and Decrease of Frequency, with Lowering of Temperature Effect of Lowering of Temperature in cadadena face of Aegis tude and Decrease of Frequency in Pulsation of Frog’s Heart Photographic Record of Pulsations of Dessmodiwm during Continuous Rise of Temperature Record of Pulsations of Geyiccean at mieent tempat: Record of Pulsations of Frog’s Heart at Different Temperatures (Pembrey and Phillips) : Effect of Cooling to Thermo-tonic Nintewdm on ene if Dacia: dium . Effect of Rapid Center & ie ead Water Photographic Record of Pulsation of Desmodium, shomine Sub- Baie. during slower Up Movement, as Nodules : Photographic Record of Cyclic Groupings in ant oucmens Palatans of Desmodium, showing Sub-pulses . Photographic Record of Autonomous eee ae af Dean, showing Hourly Period , Record showing that Rhythmic Tissue af Dia is Ine of being Tetanised , : : ° . us Curve showing Relation Hetween Pemperaiare and Period of Pulsation in Desmodium Curve showing Relation between Demeter and Pees of Palee tion in the Heart of a Frog . Simple Alternation of Pulsation in Daswbdnin Periodic Groupings of Pulsation in Desmodium . Simple Alternation of Pulsation in Frog’s Heart (Pembrey aid Phillips) b PAGE 286 288 289 403 395 XXXiV PLANT RESPONSE FIG. I5I. 152. 153: 154- 155- 156. 157. 158. 159. 160. 161. 162. 163. 164. 165. 166. 167. 168. 169. 170. 171. 172. 173. 174. 175; 176. 177. 178. 179. Effect of Barium Chloride Solution on Desmodium Arrest of Beat of Ventricle of Frog at Diastole by ‘Appliedane of Acid . = Systolic Arrest of Heir. ‘heat = Dilute NaHO Selating (Gaskell) Arrest of Desmodium Pulsation at ‘ Diastole’ by Application of Acid . - Arrest of Paesuon of Desivdiam at ee by Applicaton of Dilute Alkali 4 ‘ A 2 Diagrammatic Bepeeriece ef the Sheshunpratt : Photograph of Shoshungraph_ . - : - : ’ bie Effect of Cold on Suction < 3 : 5 ; ‘ Curve showing Normal Suction at 23° bres Teens Suction at 35° C., and the After-effect Aor 3 on Return to Normal Tem- perature . . : . Method of Piypdesseitie alarmed Record obtained by Method of Eivirosate Pawnee of re Applications of Cold and Warm Water Record, obtained by Method of Hydraulic ree of Success Effects of Cold and Warm Water. : . Effect of Strong KNO, Solution Effect of Strong NaCl Solution Effect of Copper Sulphate Solution : Record showing Recovery to be Hastened by the Tact Bi inte Activity which is caused by Application of Warm Water to the Roots . Diagrammatic Recreseneaad caf Bulenced Cae m Complete Apparatus for Crescographic Record under Ordinary aad Balanced Conditions : Multiple Growth-responses (Pedunele of Cries} Responses of Leaf of Avtocarpus to Thermal Stimulation Renewal of Growth-pulsation by Thermal Stimulus in 7% nase indica originally at Standstill . Initiation of Erectile Response in Leaf by Supply ee Water to petal Drought-rigored A/imosa Initiation of Growth-pulsation S Small eanay a Walks . Drought-rigored Seedling of Cucurbita . : - Curve showing Relation between Internal Hydrostatic Dean oa Rate of Growth (Crinum Lily) . : F Photographic Record showing the Slow Puledtions fi Large Amplinda of Desmodium Leaflet at 30° C. to become very much Quickened and Reduced in Amplitude at 42° C. Growth-pulsation seen in Seedling of Balsam . Photographic Record of Responses of Mature Style of Ditana alia to External Thermal Stimulus . Photographic Record of Responses of Style of aise alta in asad Growth had come to a Temporary Stop , : Photographic Record of Response of Growing Style of Date alba to Externa) Stimulus P / - . ; 3 : aed PAGE 351 351. 352 352 353 365 370 374 375 375 378 380 384 384 386 401 414 416 417 420 425 426 426 429 431 432 434 435 436 FIG, 180. 181. 182. 183. 184. 185. 186. 187. 188. 189. 190. I9l. 192. 193. 194. 195. 196. 197. 198. 199. 200. 201. 202. 203. 204. ILLUSTRATIONS XXXV Balanced Record of Response in Growing Peduncle of Eucharis Lily to Electrical Stimulation : : : : . - Semicircular eee Rheostat interposed in Heating Cir- cuit . . . ox Record of Growth in Cri tnum at Tee ah 34° C. and 35° C. : Thermo-crescent Curve af Gravel in tesniim aby tiles eee ously Increasing Temperature . Curve showing Relation between Tete aa Rae ae Growth, as deduced from the Thermo-crescent Curve Series of Responses of Growing Organ of Crinum Lily, Sie satis Balanced Conditions at Three Different Temperatures Curve showing the Relation between Stimulus and Response in ihe same Organ, under the two different Tonic Conditions of 30° C. (upper curve) and 37° C. (lower curve) . F A Continuous Photographic Record of Autonomous Pulsation of Des: modium gyrans from 6 P.M. to 6 A.M. Hydrometric Apparatus for Recording Gentian: arian of Rate of Growth . : : Photographic Record cigee een of Bate a Transpiration in in Cucurbita, from 3 P.M. to 12 P.M. Continuous Photographic Record of Variation a Rate of Growth in Four Days’ Old Seedling of Oryza sativa, from 3 P.M. till 9 A.M., that is during Eighteen Hours Continuous Photographic Record of yaueuon of Rate ee Groth in Seedling of Zamarindus indica, a Fortnight Old, from 3 P.M. to yAL Mey F . Effect of Es tonic nd ea on anniby Balanced Records of Effect of Ether on Growth . : Excitatory Effect of Dilute Solution of Sodium cathe: on Growth : Acceleration of Growth is ae of Suinian of Sees Spasmodic Alternations of Growth under Alcohol Unbalanced Record showing Action of Acid in Causing Rulasation and Ultimate Arrest of Growth Unbalanced Record showing the Action af Aiea a the Beta nistic Action of Subsequent Application of Acid Effect of Strong Solution of NaCl on Rate of Growth, as Modified i Different Constitutions of Specimens The Effect of Different Constitutions in Senne se pee Offered to Poisons. The Action of 5 per Cent. Solution of oes Sulphate The Effect of F hueeble fadaccd oniationa in Roabline Plant to Shake off Result of Toxic Dose of Copper Sulphate . Opposite Effects of Large and Small Doses of Poison Diagrammatic Representation showing Differential Effect of Weight on Lateral Walls of Cells Diagrammatic Representation of a Maliicelilac Greae PAGE 436 443 445 447 449 460 467 470 471 472 473 474 480 481 481 482 483 483 484 484 487 487 488 494 495 XXXVi PLANT RESPONSE 210. 2II. 214. 215. 216. Diagrammatic Representation of Experiment showing Curvature Induced by Unilateral Pressure Exerted by Particles hee Record of Responsive Curvature induced in Crznwm under Experi- mental Conditions shown in fig. 205 Record of Apogeotropic Response in Scape SS Bieta Tis Response Records showing Differences in Rate .of Curvature according as Specimen is held at Angles of 45° and 135° é 4 Diagrammatic Representation of Different Positions of a Single Cell, according as the Specimen is held at an Angle of 45° or 135°, showing Consequent Redistribution of Statoliths (after F. Darwin) 5 Effect on ApbeeonGgic titovenient a Wepicanen of tees ala Water to Upper and Lower Surfaces alternately of a Horizontally laid Crinum Lily ‘ Effect on Apogeotropic Movement of Meivorey Applications af Cold alternately to Upper and Lower Surfaces of mee laid Scape of Uried’s Lily Experimental Connections for obtaining Blectrical Responee ere to Direct and Indirect Effects of Stimulation Record of Positive Electrical Variation, indicating Positive Tinenoe Variation (represented by Down Curve), induced in Growing Region by Moderate Stimulation on same side of Tip . Record showing Galvanometric Positivity subsequently Neutralised under Transmission of True Excitatory Effect, due to Continuance of Moderate Stimulation of the Tip Record showing Neutralisation and Reversal of Electrical Remodel at Responding Region, under Strong Stimulation of Tip . Record showing Negative Electrical Response represented by Up Curve, indicating Negative Turgidity-Variation due to Direct Stimulation F Record showing peste Electrical Vacation indica pasitive Turgidity-Variation of Distal Point, under Moderate Stimulation of Proximal . The Relative Electrical phd surely Nariabions na two Diamatae ally Opposite Points when Strong and egitney -continued Stimulus is applied Mechanical Responses af Shoot a of Root to Unilateed Seitniites applied at the Tip . Mechanical Response of Root of Bindi soil to very vo Unilateral Stimulation applied at the Tip 502 504 595 515 517 517 518 518 519 521 526 527 Mechanical Responses of Peduncle of Sas and Reet of ‘Bid weed to Unilateral Thermal Stimulation at the Growing Region Diagram showing the various Responsive Effects induced at the Growing Region. ‘ ‘ 5 ° : : . . . Experimental Arrangement for obtaining Records on Smoked Drum of Responses given to Direct and Indirect Stimulation by Leaf of Wimosa Mechanical Responses of Tat de Minaka 527 529 530 531 FIG. 225. 226. 227. 228. 229. 230. 231. 232. 233- 234. Bam 236. Baye 238. 239. 240. 241. 242. 243. 244. 245. 246. 247. 248. 249. 250. 251. 252. ILLUSTRATIONS ‘XXXVil Erectile Response of Leaf of (/zmosa due to Transmission of Indirect Effect to Distal Side, when Proximal is Stimulated . Curious Response of Ar7sema ‘Curves showing Effect of Amputation on Rate af one aad Response in Root of Bindweed Response of Leaf of A/Zimosa in Favourable Tenis Candies o ~ Chemical Stimulus of 3 per Cent. Salt Solution Response of Leaf of JA/zmosa in Ordinary Tonic Condition to the Chemical Stimulus of Io per Cent. Solution of Salt . Polar Effects of Currents due to Localised Application on Upper Half of Pulvinus of Erythrina indica : Effects of Anode and Kathode on Variation of Rate a oe in Root of Bindweed exhibited by Balanced Growth-record . Responsive Curvature in Scape of Crzmum Lily by Unilateral Application of Anode and Kathode Effect in Acceleration of Rate of Growth of Sains of Or yr ae of Current through the Soil Longitudinal Contraction and Retardation a Growth aoe Light in in Hypocotyl of Szzapzs nigra . Balanced Record of Variation of Grow thes in Elewate bud 7 CH cnum Lily under Diffuse Stimulation of Light Oscillatory Response of Arsenic acted on eae by ewan ’ Radiation Multiple Response to ise af Seanad Peauet of Deus Response of Terminal Leatlet of Desmodium to Strong Light from Above Response of Wemainal Leaflet ‘of Detter, to Banleahe cea ee Below : Diagrammatic eerreentatioa on the SWaaneticglly Cantuailed Recorder Heliotropic Chamber oan Otaaesenily Controlled Recorder Heliotropic Response of Szzapzs - : Heliotropic Response of Szzafzs to Sunlight Microscope Recorder Record of Response of Root of Sinapis nigra Positive Heliotropic Movement of Terminal Leaflet ae ere Converted by Strong and too Se -continued Stimulus of Light into Oscillatory Movement . : Response of Hypocotyl of Sznap7s nigra Responses to Successive Uniform Stimuli of Thexual Beaune in Pistil of AZusa . Response of Hypocotyl of Timariwaes ailine a to Casaucne cae lation of Thermal Radiation Effect of Electric Waves on Growth : Response to Diurnal Light and Darkness of Bisinare Stem a Tpomea held vertical Response to Daily Periodic Light sat Darcie: af Piaciaecapie Siem of Cucurbita maxima . 4 : . 5 ; : = PAGE 532 533 541 551 552 555 557 558 560 572 574 575 584 586 587 589 591 593 593 599 601 604 609 616 617 618 625 626 xx xvili PLANT RESPONSE FIG. 253- Positive Heliotropic Response of Leaflet of Robinia to Sunlight Acting from Above 254. Positive Heliotropic Response af Teanes ar Rrythrina hegingt 255. Responses of JZmosa to Sunlight of not too long Dutation . 256. Response of Pulvinus of A/zmosa to Action of Continuous eo from Above 257. ~Responses of Osalis to Sunlight : 258. Negative Multiple Response in Bzophytum when Acted on for Above by Strong Sunlight 259. Different Limits of Flexibility 260. Movement of Terminal Leaflet of Daman piece itself Parallel to Incident Horizontal Light 261. Curve showing Time-relations of the Response Aubulat NMoveinent of Terminal Leaflet of Desmodium under Light, as represented in ’ the last figure . 262. Record showing that eran s is not the Percepave Osh 263. «Positive Heliotropic Movement of Leaf of AZanpifera indica eater Sunlight acting from Above 264. ‘Negative Heliotropic Response of Leaves of ar tories cue Sin- light acting from Above . . 265. Torsional Response of Terminal Leaflet of Desmoanae inde the Action of Lateral Sunlight 266. Torsion-recorder . - . 267. Torsional Response of Leaf Gr eee when Laterally Stimulated by Sunlight 268. Torsional’ Response of Leaf ' of Mimosa to Titers Chemical Stimulus 269. Torsional Response of Complex Strip of Ebonite ‘and indian Hibber under Lateral Action of Strong Radiation 270. Records showing Modification of Torsional Response unde Induced Variations of Differential Excitabilities in Pulvinus of AZimosa 271. Records showing Torsional Response to Geotropic Stimulus and Induced Modifications in Terminal Leaflet of Zxythrina indica 272. Retardation and Reversal of Normal Torsional Movement by the unilateral Action of Light in Porana paniculata 273. Photographic Record of Diurnal Movement of Petiole of Biephytum from 5 P.M. till 9 A.M. : 274. Continuous Records of the Dinrnal Movement duane Thinty- six Hours in Two Specimens of Mimosa 275. Initiation of Multiple Response in Lateral Panne of Desai originally at Standstill 276. Photographic Record of arsnemene Paleaseus in Tate Leaflet of Desmodium gyrans under Action of Sunlight, showing Periodic Reversals A 277. Model of an Artificial Plant ; 278. Diagrammatic Representation of a Windmill ith Attached eae and Accumulator PAGE 629 629 631 632 633 634 647 649 ¥ PART I SIMPLE RESPONSE CHAPTER: I THE PLANT AS A MACHINE Responsive movements in plants--Work done by plant—Plant as a machine — Indicator-diagrams — Physiological response-curves—Pulse-records—Cardia- grams—Modification of pulse by poison and other agencies—Automatic response in plants—Optical Lever Recorder—Effect of external agencies on automatic pulse-beat in plants. FROM the moment when the germinating seedling bursts its seed-coat, a complex series of movements is initiated. The radicle turns downwards, the plumule up. Underground, the root gropes its way towards moist places, and contrives to avoid hard stones and obstacles. Above ground, the stem is seen to bend, as if in search of light. Tendrils twine about a support. These, amongst many visible movements, are striking enough, but within the unruffled exterior of the plant-body there are others, energetic and incessant, which escape our scrutiny. Now all these activities are but so many expressions of the response of the plant to the various stimuli by which the living organism is constantly being excited. The plant- organ sometimes responds locally to the direct impact of stimulus, and at other times the effect is conveyed away by conducting elements analogous to nerves, and the responsive changes are manifested at a distance. Thus the most varied and important functions of plant-life are brought about by the power of the tissue to respond to stimulus, that is to say, by the irritability of the plant-cells. In such fashion, work is performed continuously by the organism, as if by a machine, and the magnitude of the work performed is often very considerable, as is seen, for instance, when sprouting seedlings break through a pavement. B PLANT RESPONSE i) The few instances enumerated by no means exhaust the activities that go on in the living machine of the plant ; they only suffice to give us a glimpse into the complexity of its functions. Wecan arrive at a comprehensive idea of such multifarious and obscure phenomena only by coming to understand the machine itself, and trying to disentangle the processes by which the various stimuli supplied by the environment bring about the appropriate responsive move- ments in the organism. This is difficult to do, inasmuch as the intricate internal machinery is hidden from our view. Indicator-diagrams.—Though the interior be thus con- cealed, however, it is still not impossible, by careful observation of external actions, to gain some conception of the hidden mechanism. Let us take, for example, the analogous case of a steam-engine. That we may be able to infer at any moment the efficiency of various hidden parts of the machine, we attach, to the moving piston, a recording apparatus, and from the diagram thus obtained we are able to judge of the working efficiency of the engine. The upstroke is followed by a downstroke, and a recording pen traces for us, on moving paper, the responsive movements. But an irregu- larity may suddenly take place in the curve. This is due to some internal obstruction. On removal of the cause, the amplitude of the record is restored, and the pulsating strokes resume their normal frequency. In dealing with living machines also, we may use similar contrivances, in order to gain some indication of their efficiency ; and by means of indicator-diagrams, or ‘response-curves,’ thus obtained, we are able to gather much information as to the physiological perfection or imperfection of the living machine. Pulse-records as indicators of physiological efficiency. We shall first take as an example that responsive pulsation with which we are most familiar, our own heart-beat. As ii the steam-engine the energy of heat brings about the responsive movement of the piston, so in the heart, some internal stimulus brings about responsive pulsations. A THE PLANT AS A MACHINE 3 sudden contractile movement is followed by relaxation. By a series of these, the blood is forced in a pulsating manner through the arteries, and we perceive the pulsatory movement at the wrist. Physicians by feeling such pulse-throbbings are able to pronounce on the condition of a patient. Or the movements may be recorded by means of a lever- arrangement, in which the short arm of the lever rests on the throbbing pulse. Its longer arm, provided with a tracing point, records these pulsatory movements on a travelling band of paper which is moved by clock-work at a uniform rate. It is on this principle that the instruments known as sphygmographs are constructed, and the response-records, or sphygmograms, reveal the physiological condition of the individual at the time (figs. 1 and 3). Fic. 1. Record of (az) Healthy Adult and (4) Senile Iluman Pulse (Broadbent) The heart-record, however, has been still more directly obtained, in the case of the lower animals, by attaching one end of the lever to the apex of the heart itself. Each con- traction and subsequent recovery is now recorded, in the manner which has been indicated. If we know the rate at which the recording surface is travelling, or if we make time- marks at regular intervals, we are able to determine the frequency of pulsation. The record also gives us the amplitude of each pulse. If now these records are to furnish reliable indications of the internal condition of the living machine, then, any circumstance which affects this internal condition must reveal itself in the external record. And this is found to be the case. For example, the effects of age are seen in the accompanying record (fig. 1); and that of poison by B2 4 PLANT RESPONSE the gradual waning of pulsation, culminating in arrest at the moment of death (fig. 2). Similarly, changes of heat and cold, and the influence of various drugs (fig. 3), are all discernible from the modifica- tions which they induce in the pulse-record. For the purpose of studying the actions by which the plant lic. 2. Effect of Muscarin in arrest- responds to the various stimuli mee of Frog’s Ventricle of its environment, I have been a sie indicates the moment of able; 10 .Seuise appara, ay application of reagent in this and means of which records of its “eae responsive pulsations may be made. In the matter of automatic pulsations, we have in plants many instances which have not hitherto been recog- nised ; but in one case which is well known, that of Desmodium gyrans—Hedysarum gyrans, the telegraph-plant—we observe pulsatory movements of its lateral leaflets, which, as I shall Fic. 3. Record of Human Pulse (a) before and (0) after Inhalation of Nitrite of Amyl. (Broadbent.) elsewhere show, exhibit a resemblance to those of the animal heart, a resemblance which is not merely superficial, but is the result of causes fundamentally the same. This telegraph-plant grows wild on the Gangetic plain, where its Indian name is Bon Charal or ‘outcast of the forests, and where the peasant belief is that it dances to the clapping of the hand. It is a papilionaceous plant with trifoliate leaves, of which the terminal leaflet is large, and the two lateral very small. Each of the latter is inserted on the petiole by means of a motile organ known as a pulvinus. THE PLANT AS A MACHINE 5 These lateral leaflets, when in normal condition, go on con- tinuously, and apparently spontaneously, executing approxi- mately up and down movements, each of which takes from two to four minutes to complete. The great difficulty in recording the pulsatory movements of Desmodium arises from the extreme slenderness of these lateral leaflets. This is such that in attaching to them a recording lever, however light, its weight, and the friction of the writing-point, are sufficient to bring their movements to a stop. I have, however, succeeded in overcoming this difficulty by devising a recording Optical Lever. This lever consists of a very light aluminium wire, or, which is still better, the stripped quill of a peacock’s tail feather, this being extremely light, and sufficiently rigid for the purpose. The two arms of the lever are unequal. The fulcrum rod rests on frictionless supports of glass or agate. The same rod carries a light mirror. A thread of cocoon silk is stuck to the motile leaflet by a minute drop of shellac varnish. The far end of the thread is looped, and fixed at any suitable notch on the arm B of the lever. The other arm of the lever has a light sliding counterpoise. It will thus be seen that by gradually shifting the silk loop nearer the fulcrum, the magnification may be increased. ‘When the automatically moving leaflet executes a downward movement, the arm B is pulled down, and there is a rotation of the fulcrum rod with its attached mirror. A spot of light reflected from the mirror is thus suddenly moved downwards from its original position. It will be observed that by moving the recording surface further away, the magnification may be still more enhanced. A wide latitude of magnifica- tion may thus be obtained, by changes in the effective length of arm of the lever, and by variation of the distance of the record- ing surface. Thus, forexample, for purposes of demonstration, with a screen at a distance of five metres, it is easy to exhibit a pulsatory movement magnified to as much as one metre in amplitude. But in the case of the illustrations in the present book, it has not been found necessary to have any magnifica- 6 PLANT RESPONSE tion whatever, since the movement of the leaflet is itself considerable. | A very light counterpoise is used, as will be seen later, to exert a slight pull on the leaflet in an upward direction when necessary, the sliding arrangement enabling us to vary the amount of this tension. It will thus be seen that the leaflet is practically free from constraint, and any movement, however slight, is easily detected. When the leaflet falls, the spot of light moves, say downwards, and vice versa. The record of the entire response—down movement followed by up—may thus be made on a vertical revolving drum, whose speed is regulated by clock-work. The magnification of the record having been determined previously, and the speed of the drum being known, the response-curve gives the absolute movement and the time-relations of such movement. Instead of using a vertical drum, it is more convenient to record on the revolving surface of a horizontal drum. The up and down movement of the spot of light may now be converted into lateral, or left and right movement, by means of a second mirror suitably inclined. The finer adjustment of the reflected spot of light may be brought about by means of a milled head with which the second mirror is provided. A photographic record may be obtained by wrapping over the drum surface a sensitised roll-film. But since these movements are comparatively slow, it is easy to obtain the record more simply by following the spot of light with a recording pen, which slides on a horizontal guide-bar, parallel with its movements. These response-records can _ be traced on a large scale in the presence ofan audience, by the use of the Demonstration Recorder, which consists of a twin-drum, over which is wrapped an endless band of paper to serve as the recording surface (fig. 4). Elastic bands pass over the two drums, one of which is kept revolving by clock-work. The excursion of the spot of light is now followed by means of a sliding ink-well, from which projects the ink-sponge. By this means, the tracing of the response-curve, and its various modifications under the pom Soe =e TEE PUAND AS AS MACHINE 7 action of different influences, can be made visible to the whole audience. It is thus possible to obtain records of these pulsatory movements, by attaching the Optical Lever to the leaflet of an intact plant. Or we may detach a petiole, | (it (3 ey 1! | bh B ars i ss aw li = " (a —_\ } | } : ieee a 7S — : os AV, zs) : Nereis. i a, > VE ait en | = 6h a be & ‘ | = Fic. 4. Demonstration Optical Pulse-Recorder B, Arm of Optical Lever, attached to moving leaflet ; L, Ray of light, which after two reflections from the two mirrors falls on the recorder ; Cc, Clock, which keeps twin-drum—on which is wrapped the recording paper—- revolving ; H, Horizontal guide-bar; kK, Ink-well, with projecting sponge. carrying the leaf, and place it in water, in which case it will remain alive as long as a couple of days, executing its accustomed pulsatory movements during a considerable time. The effect of any given agency, say poison, on the living 8 PLANT RESPONSE machinery may now be observed, as graphically indicated in the waning and final arrest of the pulse-record (fig. 5). Or the plant may be killed by passing through it excessively strong electric shocks, after which the occurrence of death will be indicated by the arrest of pulsation (fig. 6). Thus we see not only the similarity between the pulsations of Desmodium and those of cardiac muscle, but also how similarly both are affected by external agencies, such as poison. Later, we shall study the effects of other physiological in- Fic. 5. Death of Plant, and Arrest of Fic. 6. Death, and Arrest ot Pulsation, by Poison Pulsation, in Leaflet of Desmo- dium by Strong Electric Shock fluences on both. In the present chapter, however, it has been my aim to show that these pulse-records give us a reliable indication of the very obscure modifications of the life-processes initiated in the living tissues by various external factors. Speaking generally, we may say that an exciting reagent exalts the pulse, a depressing reagent reduces the amplitude of pulsation, and a poison arrests it permanently, this arrest being death. In the cases which we have chosen as examples, there is the advantage of a store of latent energy, which maintains the pulsation by providing an internal source of stimulus. This internal stimulation, as will be shown later, is really derived from external sources, the absorbed energy having been held latent in the plant. We shall in the next chapter take up a very much simpler case, in which the plant has no such reserve, but responds immediately to external stimulus. THE PLANT AS A MACHINE 9 SUMMARY A plant, like a machine, responds either to the impact of external forces, or to energy that is latent within. As the working efficiency of an engine is exhibited by indicator-diagrams, so the physiological efficiency of a living machine may be inferred from the character of its pulse- records. Agencies which depress the physiological condition of a tissue, also depress its responsive pulsation. At the death of a tissue there is a permanent arrest of pulsation. CHART 7a MECHANICAL RESPONSE TO STIMULUS Molecular derangement caused by stimulus—Expressicn in change of form, contraction— Mechanical model—Myograph— Response by differential con- traction in pulvinated plant-organs— Longitudinal response in plants— Response of plant to all forms of stimulus—Plant chamber—Practicable forms of graduated stimulus—Electro-thermic stimulator-—Stimulation by condenser discharge—Response-recorder—Advantage of counterpoise—Response of Brophytum to thermal stimulation— Response to condenser discharge—Absolute measurements of motile effect and of work performed—Effect of load—Definite determination of threshold of response—Determination of variation of excita- bility by measurement of minimally effective stimulus. FEW of the phenomena of plant-life are so striking as the conspicuous mechanical movements of certain plants, like Mimosa, commonly known as ‘sensitive’ in contradistinction to ‘ordinary’ plants. These movements take place in response to various forms of stimulation, such as is caused by mechanical touch or application of heat. It will be shown, however, in the course of the present book that this division of plants into sensitive and ordinary is arbitrary, since all plants are sensitive—that is to say, react to stimulus. The plant, throughout its life, is constantly responding to stimuli, external and internal. Some of its responses are manifested in mechanical movements which are too striking to be over- looked. Others, not so obvious, have passed hitherto un- noticed. But in both these cases changes of form occur in the tissue, in consequence of stimulation. In some instances, owing to conditions which will be explained later, these changes produce little visible effect. In others, the responsive change of form is displayed in a striking manner, owing to certain advantageous circumstances of structure, and to the possession of a magnifying arrangement. —— MECHANICAL RESPONSE TO STIMULUS [I - The shock of stimulus causes molecular derangement in the tissue of the plant, and it is this fundamental mole- cular change that finds expression in mechanical movement. It finds independent expression also in electrical move- ment. For the conspicuous display of mechanical response certain peculiar structural arrangements are, as has been said, advantageous; but for the exhibition of electrical response, the molecular change itself, which is concomitant to excitation, is the only condition. This subject of the electrical response of plants, however, I treat in detail else- where.' For the present we are concerned only with the question of mechanical response to stimulus. We have not only to determine the existence of such response, but also to ascertain under what conditions it occurs,and by what means it is brought about. The whole sequence of molecular events initiated by stimulus and expressed as mechanical response, may be very simply illustrated by means of an india-rubber model. We take a piece of stretched india-rubber, attached to a recording lever. Therubber is enclosed in a tube in which there is also enclosed a spiral of thin German-silver wire, by which the india- rubber may be subjected to the momentary action of heat. The quantity of heat generated is regulated by the strength ~ and duration of an electrical current flowing through the heating wire. This application may be uniform for successive experiments, or increased at will. Longitudinal response.— The thermal stimulus causes a molecular rearrangement in the substance of the india-rubber, in consequence of which the piece becomes shorter and broader. This sudden longitudinal shortening is recorded by the lever as the first half of the responsive movement. As the substance gradually recovers from the effect of the momentary stimulation, the molecules return to their normal position, with a concomitant restoration of the india-rubber to its original form. During this second half of the process, we ' Bose, Response in the Living and Non-Living (Messrs. Longmans, Green & Co.). Bose, Electro-Physiology of Plants. 12 PLANT RESPONSE obtain the curve of recovery.’ If we apply similar stimuli successively, we obtain successive responses which are alike (fig. 7). But if stronger stimulus be applied, by means of stronger heating current, the amplitude of response will be correspondingly increased. In the simple instance which we have considered, the response-record was obtained by taking advantage of the sudden contraction of the india- rubber. In the response of con- tractile animal muscle, we obtain response-records in exactly the same manner (fig. 8), and such records are known as myographs (fig. 9). Similar contraction in length, or LONGITUDINAL RESPONSE under the action of stimulus, has been shown byl Pfeffer to occur in the Fic. 8. Mechanical Lever Recorder The muscle M with the attached bone is securely held at one end, the other end being con- nected with the writing lever. Under the action of stimulus the contracting muscle pulls the lever, and moves the tracing point to the right over the travelling recording surface Pp. When the muscle recovers from contraction the eel » ane mga gy 19-17 > 5 “ e Fic. 7. Response of India-rubber tracing point returns to its Thermal stimulus for 1 second at original position. See on P intervals of two minutes. the record of muscle-curve. filament of the sensitive stamens of Cyneree@. 1 shall, how- ever, show in Chapter IV. that such longitudinal contraction under stimulus is not unique, but a phenomenon very exten- sively exhibited by plant-tissues, as seen in the series of uniform responses to stimulation, obtained from the stamen of an ordinary plant, which is here given (fig. 10). ' Such models made of catgut and stretched caoutchouc have been used by Engelmann for explaining muscle response. MECHANICAL RESPONSE TO STIMULUS 13 Differential response.—But the responsive movement in plants is more generally produced by differential contractile movement, and a mechanical model again will clearly show how such movements are brought about. We take two equal strips of unequally contracting substances, which are glued together throughout their length. The two strips consist of ebonite and _ the relatively more contractile caoutchouc. Fic. 10. Photographic Record of Longitudinal Contractile _ Kesponse in ordinary Stamens (Grownta ariza) Fic. 9. Series of Contractile Responses in Muscle If such a compound strip be -held horizontally, with the more contractile element below, and if we subject it to thermal stimulation in the manner described above, the result will be a responsive curvature downwards, the more contractile caoutchouc forming the concave surface. Thermal Fic. 11. Differential Lateral Response of Compound Strip Thermal stimuli applied at intervals of three minutes. stimulus may be applied, as in the last case, by sending a momentary heating current through an enclosing spiral of German-silver wire, the responses being recorded in the usual manner (fig. I1). In such cases, where the upper and lower elements are 14 PLANT RESPONSE unequally contractile, we obtain a DIFFERENTIAL RESPONSE, the more contractile becoming concave; and it is evident that such movements must take place in a direction perpen- dicular to the plane of separation. Typical cases of mechanical response in plants are obtained from pulvinated organs. A good example of this is found at the insertion of the petiole in Wzmosa pudtca. When such an organ is: stimulated, it is the lower half that undergoes the greater contraction, and the leaf is depressed by the concavity thus produced. It is generally assumed that the upper half of the pulvinus is not excitable, but this, as I shall show later, is an error. The responsive movement, however, is due to the differential contraction of the two halves, and, as already explained, takes place in a direction perpendicular to the plane which separates them. Such differential response will be found characteristic of all organs possessing dorsi-ventral differentiation. Whenever the plant is subjected to any sudden dis- turbance, the sensitive leaf reacts by a fall, which is brought about by the hinge-like mechanism at the pulvinus. The sudden disturbance which induces the fall constitutes the stimulus. The leaf responds when it is shaken, or cut, or when a prick is applied to it, or when a sudden variation of temperature is produced, as by touching it with a hot wire, or with ice, or when an electrical shock is passed through it, or if it be acted on by certain chemical reagents, or a beam of strong light be thrown on it. All these constitute the various forms of stimuli—mechanical, thermal, electrical, chemical, and photic. We have next to study the relation between the intensity of the stimulus and the extent of response under varying conditions; that is to say, we have to determine the ‘threshold of response, in other words, the minimum intensity of stimulus that will be just sufficient to initiate reaction. We have then to observe the repeated response of the plant to repeated stimulation, whether uniform or gradually increasing. We have to detect the signs of fatigue if there be any, and ——————E—————r MECHANICAL RESPONSE TO STIMULUS 15 discover after what period of rest this disappears. We have also to record the exact time-relations of these phenomena. And further we have to study the effects of various external agencies in modifying the response. In order to carry out these investigations, it will be necessary first to arrange for placing the plant under suitable conditions for experiment. The next point is the devising of facilities for applying a stimulus of known intensity, which can be repeated, or increased by definite amounts, at will. And, lastly, there must be some means of obtaining an exact record of the response, from which the absolute movement of the responding organ and its time-relations may be deduced. Experimental plant chamber.—As regards the first of these, it is advisable to have a special plant chamber within which the specimen can be subjected to the necessary con- ditions. This chamber may consist of a base-board and a movable cover. The framework of the latter is of wood, with glass panes. In order to give easy access to the plant during experiment, one side of the cover has a hinged window. The recording Optical Lever is placed inside the chamber, and the glass cover protects the recorder from any accidental disturbance caused by air-currents. In connection with this, it is also important to provide arrangements for producing changes of temperature, and maintaining the changed condition uniform, for the required length of time. This is most satisfactorily accomplished by means of a heating coil placed inside the chamber, the temperature being regulated by suitable adjustment of the electrical current, sent into the coil through proper electrodes. Other necessary accessories consist of appliances for the purpose of stimulation, and facilities by which a constant current can be made to flow through the tissue, in experiments on the effect of electric currents on the excitability of plants. Details regarding these will be given later. The plant may be maintained in favourable humid conditions by placing wet blotting-paper inside the chamber (fig. 12). 16 PLANT RESPONSE The most important question with regard to the application of suitable stimulus is, as has been said, that it should be capable of exact measurement, of uniform repetition, and of definite increase or decrease at will. Another point which must be borne in mind is that the application of stimulus should not, by causing injury, change the excitability of the organ. As, moreover, a magnified record of the responsive movement is to be made immediately after the application, any stimulus which causes the slightest jar must necessarily Fic. 12. Plant Chamber and Recorder The glass cover is not shown. be avoided. And for these reasons the mechanical form of stimulation is inappropriate to the investigation. The three most perfect modes of stimulation which I have been able to render practicable are, then, the thermal, the electrical, and the stimulus of light. The action of the last will be described in detail in another chapter, and we shall for the present confine our attention to the first two. Electro-thermic stimulator.—Thermal stimulus may be applied very easily by touching the plant with a hot wire, but MECHANICAL RESPONSE TO STIMULUS yf it is difficult by this means to ensure the uniformity of successive stimuli, inasmuch as the wire cannot be heated repeatedly to the same temperature, or made to touch the same point, many times in succession, with an equally effec- tive contact. This difficulty is removed by means of what I have named the ELECTRO-THERMIC STIMULATOR. This consists of a thin M-shaped wire of platinum, with thick copper leads. It is slipped over the petiole which carries the sensitive leaflets. By now sending through it a current of definite intensity and duration, we can raise its temperature to any point we wish, and thus secure the application of a known intensity of stimulus at will. -The elasticity due to the peculiar form of the thermal stimulator gives a definite and constant pressure of contact (fig. 13). The observer ap- plies the stimulus with his left hand, by press- ing a_ tapping-key which is interposed in an electric circuit, for a definite short time: With his right hand ,he records on the revolving drum the exact moment of this application. This mode of thermic stimulation is, as will be shown presently, very efficient. Electric stimulation.—I have been able, however, to employ a mede of stimulus still more perfect, that, namely, of the electrical discharge from a condenser. Other forms of elec- trical stimulation may be used, such as those given by means of constant or induction currents. But these are liable, not only to cause more or less permanent internal changes by polarisation, but also to induce fatigue of the tissue. It will be shown in a later chapter that, on making the circuit, excita- tion takes place at the point where the current leaves the tissue—that is to say, at the kathode—and not at the anode, or point of entrance. By appropriate connections shown in the diagram (fig. 14), the point to be excited can be made C Tic, 13. The Electro-thermic Stimulator 18 PLANT RESPONSE kathode during ‘ charge, when the key is pressed. When the key is released, the circuit is ‘discharged,’ and the current flows in the opposite direction. The given point B is, as has been said, excited by being made kathode at the moment of charge. The immediately succeeding discharge produces no exciting effect, but it wipes off any residual polarisation effect caused by charge. The plant-tissue is thus maintained in as completely normal a Fic. 14. Diagram of Connections for Stimulation by Condenser Discharge Pressure of key K charges the condenser c through the plant. Release of key brings it in contact with M, discharging the condenser through the plant. L, responding leaflet attached to recording lever by thread s. condition as possible. The excitation produced in the plant by current to or from the condenser, I shall, for simplicity, designate as ‘stimulation by condenser discharge.’ In the course of the present chapter we shall study the response of the leaf of J/zosa, shown by its fall, and also that of other sensitive plants, exhibited by the closure of the leaflets, as in the case of Azophytum sensitivum. One difficulty encountered in obtaining successive responses, in these latter cases, was due to the fact that the responding MECHANICAL RESPONSE TO STIMULUS 19 leaflets, after each downward response, would sometimes remain persistently closed, for an indefinite period, thus pre- venting the continuation of the experiment. In cases where the leaflets are completely closed, one naturally regards the position as one of fatigue, or complete insensitiveness, because no further mechanical response is then obtainable. This depressed position, however, may not be indicative of total want of sensibility, for the apparent absence of response may really be due to the fact that further closure of the leaflets is a mechanical impossibility. We may consider an analogous instance in the case of animal tissues, muscle floating in mercury for example. The tissue remains per- sistently contracted after a single stimulus, and further response is impossible. But if, again, the muscle be stimu- lated while under tension, it responds to each stimulation, the process of recovery being aided by the external tension. Practicai importance of counterpoise.—Acting on this idea, it appeared to me that if we applied an external tension, the restoration of the leaflet to the natural outspread position might be helped, and the difficulty solved of ob- taining the uniform repetition of effects of successive stimuli at brief and regular intervals of time. I therefore placed a small sliding counterpoise on that arm of the lever which was not attached to the leaflet. This was found to fulfil its purpose. For in observing the effects of successive stimuli on different leaflets, I found that while neighbouring leaflets, not under tension, closed up after a few stimulations, and gave no further response, the leaflet which was attached to the lever, and which was under some slight tension, recovered its normal outspread position in the course of three to five minutes, and continued to respond in a normal manner to a long series of successive stimuli. We shall now proceed to observe the actual responses obtained. The object here is not to investigate the peculiar or specific reaction of any one sensitive plant in particular, but the effects found universally among motile plant-organs. The occurrence of such effects in plants exhibiting all C2 20 PLANT RESPONSE degrees of mechanical sensibility—from those in which it is shown in an extreme degree, to others again in which it is apparently almost non-existent—will be demonstrated in this and succeeding chapters. In order to study the responsive movements of plants, we may take either the leaflets or the main petiole of Mimosa pudica. The leaflets, however, in this case are so excessively sensitive that even the contact for experimental adjustment is sufficient to produce a closure from which they do not recover for a considerable time. The pulvinus of the main petiole, on the other hand, is considerably less sensitive. Of intermediate sensibility are the leaflets of Azophytum sensitivum, which on the whole furnish the most suitable specimens for the general purposes of these experiments. This plant, which is known to be sensitive, grows in a wild state near Calcutta, and is so common as to be considered a weed, It is a low-growing herb, with simply pinnate leaves, each bearing from ten to sixteen pairs of leaflets. A better specimen could hardly perhaps be found for the exhibition of some of the most important characteristics of mechanical response. It is not, under ordinary conditions, excessively sensitive. A gentle touch does not, as a rule, produce the closing effect, but under specially favourable circumstances its sensitiveness may equal, if not surpass, that of the A7zmosa leaflets. The closing of the leaflets takes place not upwards as in J/zmosa, but in the downward direction. I shall presently give details of the response obtained with Biophytum. But as this plant is not universally obtainable, and as it flourishes only for a short season, during and after our tropical rains, it may be best first to give an account of experiments made on the more generally accessible leaf of Mimosa. Response of Mimosa.—As the responsive movement of the leaf of Mimosa is of considerable extent, no magnifica- tion is necessary for the record. Indeed, on the contrary, for the illustrations in the present work, the records had fre- quently to be taken on a reduced scale. This was accomplished MECHANICAL RESPONSE TO STIMULUS 21 by attaching the leaf to the long arm of the recording Optic Lever, and shortening the distance of the recording surface. The records given in figs. 15 and 16 were automatically obtained by the impression of the moving spot of light on a sensitive film wrapped about the recording drum, The leaf was excited by a single strong induction-shock. In order to obtain the complete curve of response and _ recovery—the double process being accom- plished in the course of about seven minutes —the first record was taken on a slowly moving Fic. 715. Photographic Record of Response and Recovery of AZzmosa, taken on a slowly moving drum. Record shows actual movement reduced to one-third. drum. For the detailed study of the characteristic time- relations of the first part of the curve, again, two more records were obtained, one with a moderate (fig. 15) and the Fic. 16. Photographic Record of Response in different specimen, taken on a faster-n:oving drum, showing only first part of thé curve. Each division of time-scale = *5 second. other with a rapid speed of drum (fig. 110). The last of these enables us to obtain time-measurements which are accurate to less than = of a second. The method by oVU 22 PLANT RESPONSE which these rapid records are obtained will be described in Chapter XXII. 1 From records obtained on a fast-moving drum, with a fairly average specimen of J/zmosa, it is found that the leaf does not respond to stimulus immediately, there being a latent period of ;24, of a second before it begins to move. The maximum fall is attained in the course of 2 seconds after the shock. After attaining the position of maximum depression, the leaf remains in its contracted position for a further period of about thirty seconds. It then begins slowly to recover, and perfect recovery takes place in the further course of six minutes. The record given was obtained from the leaf of a plant which was one year old, and in the summer season. It will be remembered that these responsive curves are modified by the physiological condition of the plant ; thus, for example, the time taken by the leaf of a vigorous young plant for recovery may be as short as four minutes, whereas an older specimen in winter may require as long a period as eighteen minutes. We may thus obtain from the record an idea of the physiological condition of the specimen. By means of photography the taking of the record is made extremely simple, but there are certain disadvantages insepar- able from this method, which render the devising of other means essential. Forexample, the motile sensibility of plants like Mimosa and Biophytum is profoundly modified in dark- ness. In the case of the records given, the plants have been kept outside in the light, and brought in immediately before experiment. But even then, after remaining in the dark for half an hour or so, the leaves of J/zmosa become abnormally erected, till it can hardly be believed that the plant is sensi- tive, for it often becomes irresponsive to the hardest blow. Biophytum \eaflets, again, in the same circumstances undergo closure. For these reasons, long-continued experiments in a dark room are an impossibility. Various sensitive plants, again, flourish only for a short-lived season, and during that period some hundreds of experiments have to be carried out. MECHANICAL RESPONSE TO STIMULUS 2S This necessitates some method of record more expeditious than that of photography. Fortunately, the responsive movements of ieee sensitive organs are relatively slow, usually requiring several minutes for completion. And it is quite easy to foilow the excursion of the responding spot of light, with the recording pen, on a horizontal drum. There are some few special investigations, such as those on exceedingly short latent periods, in which automatic records by photography are a necessity, but for the majority of the records the second method is all that is required. By the latter means, moreover, we overcome the serious difficulty occasioned by the variation of sensibility which the plant undergoes when kept long in a dark chamber. When the second method is employed, the specimen may be placed in a well-lighted and well-ventilated room, and under these conditions it is found to maintain its sensitiveness unchanged for a considerable length of time. The fact that the spot of light reflected on the drum becomes inconspicuous in the surrounding daylight is overcome by placing in front of the recording drum a special hood with a long horizontal] slit. The back of this hood curves over the head of the observer, and the spot of light then appears very bright. Response of Biophytum.—I shall next deal with the responses obtained from zophytum. In fig. 17 are given two successive responses to two successive thermal stimuli. It will be noticed how uniform these responses are. The up- curve represents the fall of the leaflets, and the subsequent down-curve of the response exhibits its gradual return to the normal outspread horizontal position. An abnormal erectile twitch will be noticed at the beginning of each of these responses. This effect is usually present when a stimulus of whatever nature is applied at a distance from the responding leaflet. Its cause will be explained later. It should be stated here that stimulus: was in this case applied at a distance of 35 mm. from the responsive leaflet, and that the true excitatory reaction, by the depression of the leaflet, took place fifteen seconds after the application. In 24 PLANT RESPONSE other words, the excitation travelled the intervening distance with a speed of 2°3 mm. per second. The abnormal erectile Fic. 17. Response of Bzophytum to Thermal Stimulation Stimulus was applied at some distance from the responding leaflet. Thick dot represents moment of application of stimulus. “ s Fic. 18. Response of Biophytum to Electric Stimulation Ordinate represents absolute Stimulus was applied directly on the pulvinus. movement in mm. effect, however—due, as will be explained later, to hydrostatic disturbance—took place almost instantaneously. MECHANICAL RESPONSE TO STIMULUS 25 The next figure (fig. 18) gives successive responses of Biophytum to condenser discharge, the pulvinus of the leaflet being directly excited. It will be noticed that in this case of direct stimulation, no abnormal erectile twitch is present. From the magnification of the record the absolute value of the movement is known, and in the present case it was 1°88 mm. The force exerted by the leaflet during its responsive movement was found equivalent to that exerted by the weight of 17 milligrammes. The total work performed by the leaflet during each responsive movement is therefore nearly equal to 1,600 millimetre-milligrams. Effect of load.—In order to observe the effect of load on the response-curve, I added a slight additional counter- poise to the other arm of the lever, The record (fig. 19, a) shows the response-curve when the acting load is the weight of the lever; (4) shows the effect of the additional load. It will be seen that while the height of the responses was diminished, yet the period of recovery was very much reduced, from five minutes to “ues y eit oe _ coe ‘ a) without, and (@) with, less than three, under the increased additional load load. Isometric record.—The method of observing response employed in the foregoing results was that of recording the movement of the leaf. Similar methods are known in Animal Physiology as zsotonic. There is, however, an interesting method corresponding to that known in Animal Physiology as the zsometric—where, in obtaining records of responses, actual movement is almost abolished. The contraction of the more excitable half of the pulvinus exerts a certain tension, or pull. The object, under the isometric method of experiment, is to obtain records of varying responsive ten- sions of excited tissues, the physical movement being at the same time restrained. This I have been able to accomplish, 26 PLANT RESPONSE in the case of plant response, by the construction of a spiral spring-recorder, the movement of whose index is approxi- mately proportional to the tension: The recorder is constructed ofa fine flattened spiral spring. Springs of this description have the peculiarity that, when they are stretched by tension, the free end of the spiral rotates round the axis of the spring. A slight rotation may be magnified by means of a mirror and reflected spot of light (fig. 20), hid This arrangement is specially ad = appropriate to the leaf-stalk of 7 Mimosa, where the pull exerted Fic. 20. The Spiral Spring- by the excited leaf is consider- _ Beeovler able. Fig. 21 gives the isometric The leaf is practically prevented : : from moving. The tension !eSPOnse of Mzmosa. exerted by excited leaf causes Minimally effective stimulus rotation of index or mirror. - . in Biophytum.—It is well to mention here that at least in the case of Bzophytum the minimal intensity of stimulus necessary to cause response is very well defined. With a certain specimen for example, when the plant was excited by the dis- charge from a ‘OI micro- farad. condenser, charged to seven volts, there was no response. But when the condenser was charged to nine volts the discharge always produced a largé and definite —_ response. Charging of the condenser to nine and seven volts alternately would in the one case produce response, and in the other none. If now, by the action of an external agency, the Fic. 21. Isometric Response of Mimosa MECHANICAL RESPONSE TO STIMULUS 27. excitability of the tissue be increased, the seven-volt con- denser charge, before inadequate, will become adequate. Conversely, if by the action of an external agent the excita- bility of the tissue be depressed, the nine-volt charge, which was formerly effective, will become now ineffective. I find, for instance, that lowering of the temperature will, by in- creasing molecular sluggishness, reduce excitability. Hence a minimally effective stimulus becomes ineffective when the tissue is cooled. Conversely, a rise of temperature produces the reverse effect, namely, increase of excitability. This was seen in a particular experiment with Azophytum, where the minimally effective stimulus necessary at 30° C. was found to be reduced to two-thirds when the temperature was raised to 35° C. Having thus obtained a reliable stimulus, whose value may be measured with precision, and which is capable of being repeated, and having also discovered an arrangement by which the effect of a given stimulus is invariably exhibited by a uniform response-record, we are now in a position to attack various physiological problems, as regards the influence of given external agencies on the conductivity and excitability of the plant-tissue. SUMMARY Longitudinal responses are given by radial organs. A differential response, causing lateral movement, is given by an organ in which the excitability of one half is different from that of the other; and the movement takes place in a direction perpendicular to the plane of separation of the two halves. Such responses are characteristic of dorsi- ventral plant-organs. In the responses of the sensitive organs of plants we notice: a short latent period; a period during which the excitatory movement attains its maximum; and a period of slow recovery. When the stimulus is applied at a distance, a preliminary abnormal erectile twitch is occasionally observed, which is 28 PLANT RESPONSE due to hydrostatic disturbance. The true excitatory response takes place later. Besides the isotonic response, obtained by recording the actual movements of the excited leaf, it is also possible to record the isometric response where the movement is restrained, and the variation of tension caused by the con- traction of tissue is alone recorded. The intensity of a minimally effective stimulus in the case of Bzophytum is definite. This value undergoes appro- priate variation with the variation of excitability of the organ. CEAPT ER Tit ON THE UNIVERSALITY OF SENSITIVENESS IN PLANTS AS DEMONSTRATED BY MEANS OF ELECTRICAL RESPONSE Arbitrary classification of plants into sensitive and ordinary—Method of electro- motive variation for detecting state of excitation—Hydraulic model—Excita- tion of vegetable tissue, like that of animal tissue, induces galvanometric negativity—Methods of direct and transmitted excitation—Electrical and mechanical response alike record molecular derangement and recovery— Similarities in simultaneous record of mechanical and electrical response— True excitation has a concomitant negative turgidity-variation, negative mechanical response or fall, and galvanometric negativity—These are true physiological responses, and are abolished at death—Abnormal positive mechanical and electrical responses brought about by positive turgidity- variation — Direct and indirect effects of stimulation—Discrimination of differ- ences of excitability by electric test—Excitability of plant-tissues in general— Responsive power characteristic of matter. : WE have seen that when stimulus is applied to a sensitive organ like the pulvinus of J/zmosa there is a fall of the leaf, which fall is due to the excitatory contraction of the more excitable lower half of the pulvinus. Ordinary plants are said to give no motile indications, hence they are usually regarded as insensitive.’ It is difficult, however, to conceive that while the protoplasm of certain plants reacts to stimulus, that of others should not do so, On the other hand, it may be that the absence of mechanical response in these ordinary plants is not due to any want of excitability, but rather to the fact that conditions favourable to the conspicuous exhibition of motile effects do not in 1 Vines has already drawn attention to the possibility of error here: We must be careful not to assume that irritability is restricted to growing and to motile organs. For all we know to the contrary, it is possessed by the proto- plasm of all pliant organs, and if in any case the action of a stimulus is not followed by a responsive movement, we must, before we assume the absence of irritability, assure ourselves that the structure of the organ is such that a move- ment is a mechanical possibility.’—Vines, Physzology of Plants, 1886, p. 372. ~ 30 PLANT RESPONSE such cases exist. What these conditions are will be detailed in the next chapter, where it will also be shown that excita- tion of an organ may take place, even where there is little mechanical indication of the fact, owing: to antagonistic and balanced contractions. Electrical response.—It is my intention, in the course of the present work, to offer a complete demonstration of all the phenomena of excitation in plants, by means of mechanical response alone. But the conclusions to which we shall be led by the study of this response will receive irrefragable support, if they can also be established independently by some mode of investigation altogether different. Such a mode of in- quiry, namely the electrical, and the conclusions to which it leads, will be fully described in the companion volume to this work, on the Electro-Physiology of Plants. Meanwhile it is convenient in this place to enter upon a short elucidation of the principle of that method, in order that we may be able, while considering the results of mechanical response, to make casual references to confirmatory results of independent ob- servations obtained by means of the electrical method. It has been said that under the action of stimulus excited cells undergo contraction, and that owing to the consequent expulsion of water, the turgidity of the tissue is diminished. Thus one expression of the molecular change induced by stimulus is a negative variation of turgidity. But this molecular change may also be detected by means of other concomitant physical changes. For instance, the electrical level or potential of a given point may, owing to the ex- citatory molecular change, undergo variation, relatively to another point which is unexcited. A hydraulic model will serve to make this point clear (fig. 22). Let us imagine a flexible pipe of india-rubber, with bent ends of glass-tube, filled with water, and held in the middle by aclamp c. It is also supported in the stable horizontal position by spiral springs. If a single blow, say upwards, be now given to the end A, the level of the pipe at that end will be raised, and there will be a resultant flow of water from UNIVERSALITY OF SENSITIVENESS IN PLANTS 31 A to B, or away from the struck end. The intensity of the current is determined by the height to which the struck end A has been raised, and this again depends on the intensity of the blow. Hence the intensity of the current is a measure of the intensity of the stimulus or disturbance. The flow subsides with the return of the pipe to its equilibrium posi- tion. Ifthe pipe had been disturbed throughout, the level would have been raised equally at both ends, and there would have been no flow. The object of the clamp is, therefore, to confine the disturbance to one side. If the blow had been (Tea —— Fic. 22. Hydraulic Model for Explanation of Electric Response When an upstroke is given to A, a responsive current flows from A to B, and wice versa. given on the B side, the direction of the responsive flow would have been reversed. The principle of electromotive response in plants is exactly similar to this. The plant tissue is clamped at C (fig. 23), and a stimulus is given at one end, say A. The electrical level of that side is now found to be raised, it becomes electro-positive, or like the copper in a voltaic com- bination. The responsive current thus flows in the tissue from A to B, or away from the excited point. In the external circuit containing the galvanometer, it flows, of 32 PLANT RESPONSE course, in the opposite direction, that is from B to A.' The excited point A is thus electro-positive, but in physiological text-books it has been ambiguously termed negative. In order therefore to keep touch with the older terminology and yet avoid the implied error, I shall refer to the excited point as ‘ zalvanometrically negative.’ If the B end of the specimen be now excited, the direction of the responsive current will be reversed. With greater intensity of stimulus the electrical response will be found correspondingly increased. The record of such responses is (2) (0) obtained ona revolving drum, A by following the deflection of a spot of light reflected from the galvanometer mirror in a manner precisely similar to that employed with the Optic Lever (fig. 24). Current of response when Method of transmitted A ts stimulated > : . = Current of response when B stimulation.—In the simple B is stimulated <— : ° : Fic. 23. Electrical Response in Plant Case just described, the tissue bys Mictho te rocl: was stimulated directly and (a) pee is clamped at c, between locally at thetee A, hay ts (4) Responses obtained by alternately torsional vibration. There is, stimulating the two ends. Stimula- tion of A produces upward response, however, another method of and that of B downward. eames stimulation, by which a stimulus,—say by application of cut, or hot wire, or electrical shock,—is given at a point X some distance away, say to the right. The excitation now travels with a velocity characteristic of the specimen, and when it reaches the proximal electrode produces galvanometric negativity of that point. The interval of time which elapses between the application of stimulus and response will therefore depend on the velocity of transmission and the distance of the point ' The failure to understand this point clearly has been the source of many grave errors in some physiological text books. From the fact that the current in the external circuit is seen to flow in the direction of A, it has been erroneously supposed that that point is negative, or zinc-like. See Bose, Xesponse in the Living and Non- Living. UNIVERSALITY OF SENSITIVENESS IN PLANTS 33 of application. It will also be remembered that the state of excitation is attended by an expulsion of water, or negative turgidity-variation. After causing galvanometric negativity of the proximal contact, the excitation may reach the distal, and bring about reversal of response, thus constituting a _ AE Fic, 24. Electric Response Recorder diphasic variation. If, however, the distal point be very far, the excitation may by transmission through the long tract become so enfeebled as to produce practically no effect at Fic. 25. Method of Transmitted Stimulation Stimulus applied to the right at x. Excitation reaches right contact first, causing galvanometric negativity of the point. that point, in which case we obtain only the monophasic response of the proximal point. Simultaneous mechanical and electrical events, ensuing on excitation.—We may prove that these electrical responses are undoubtedly signs of excitation, by choosing D 34 PLANT RESPONSE for the electrical experiment a plant in which the state of excitation is independently manifested by mechanical response. If now these electrical and mechanical responses be indeed only two different expressions of the same thing—that is to say, of a molecular disturbance and recovery which is concomitant to excitation and recovery from excitation— then we should expect that, on taking a simultaneous record, the two responses would be shown to be initiated at the same moment, and to bear some general resemblance to each other. In the following record it will be seen that this is found to be the case (fig. 26). ; To recapitulate : let us take the concrete example of the Mimosa \eaf. When the pulvinus is excited, owing to the Fic. 26. Simultaneous Mechanical (M) and Electrical (E) Responses in Bzophylum These responses are seen to take place at the same moment. molecular change induced by stimulus, there is an expul- sion of water, or negative turgidity-variation, and in the absence of restraint this is attended by the normal negative mechanical response, or fall of the leaf. If, now, electrical connections have been made, one with the pulvinus, and the other with a distant point on the stem, it will be found that the excitatory change is attended by a strictly concomitant electrical change, the current of response flowing away from the excited point, which in other words becomes galvano- metrically negative. All these events will perhaps be more easily realised if we remember that excitation, in the typical case of Mzmosa UNIVERSALITY OF SENSITIVENESS IN PLANTS 35 gives rise simultaneously to (@) contraction of the cells, with concomitant negative turgidity-variation ; (4) negative me- chanical response, or fall of the leaf; and (c) galvanometric negative variation. Had the leaf been physically restrained by any means, the mechanical response would have been prevented, although the negative turgidity-variation con- comitant to excitation would have taken place just the same. But this internal change would have been imperceptible, and in that case we could still have detected the effect of excita- tion by means of the electromotive response. As a matter of fact it is found that the electrical response always takes place in answer to effective stimulation, even in cases where the mechanical response is rendered impossible. Wethus see that galvanometric negativity is a certain indication of the excitatory contraction of a cell, whether or not the effect of such contraction be outwardly manifested by mechanical movement. The detection of the state of excitation by the electric test is thus unfailing, and of universal application. By the employment of this electric mode of investigation, ] have shown that not sensitive plants alone, but every plant, and also every organ of every plant, is excitable. True excitatory negative versus hydrostatic positive variation.—It has been supposed that the galvanometric negativity consequent on stimulation may be due to mechani- cal movement of water in the tissue. But I have shown that this cannot be the case. For while it is true that the pro- duction of water-movement by sudden forcing of water into a tissue does cause electrical variation, yet it must be noted that the sign of this electrical change is always one of galvanometric positivity, which is opposite to that of the true excitatory response. The intensity of the true negative electrical response, moreover, varies with the physiological activity of the tissue, and is abolished with its death. The electrical variation due to mere water-movement, however, may take place even in a dead tissue, and is, as has been said, of positive sign. | If a piece of living tissue be subjected to dzrect stimula- D2 306 PLANT RESPONSE tion—that is to say, be locally disturbed, say by torsional vibration—two effects will be produced: first, the negative turgidity-variation, which is the true excitatory effect, with its attendant negative electrical variation ; and, second, the electrical effect due to hydrostatic disturbance or water- movement, which is positive. Of these two opposed electrical effects, the first, or true excitatory variation, is generally speaking much the stronger. It therefore completely masks the second, or effect of water-movement, and the resultant response is the normal negative variation. The water-move- ment effect may, however, be unmasked by killing the tissue, and then applying the same torsional vibration as before. The result is now a positive electrical response. Or the positive effect may be made to exhibit itself separately, under favourable conditions in a living tissue, by the method of indirect stimulation, that is to say, by the application of stimulus at a distance. When such a distant point is stimulated, there is a sudden expulsion of water from that point, due to stimulation. This gives rise to a wave of increased hydrostatic pressure (with its attendant positive turgidity-variation), which travels with a relatively great velocity. The true excitatory variation, travelling at its slower rate, reaches any given distant point much later. The two effects ought thus to be divided from each other by some interval of time. We should therefore expect on stimulation of a sensitive plant to find the hydrostatic disturbance, with its attendant positive fturgidity-variation—reaching the distant motile organ the earlier of the two. And since the negative turgidity-variation due to excitation causes a fall of the leaf, the positive turgidity-variation due to hydrostatic disturbance should be expected to produce an abnormal positive or erectile movement, and the same positive turgidity-varia- tion should also find a simultaneous electrical expression in the abnormal positive response. The true excitatory response—with its attendant negative turgidity-variation— should cause, later the normal negative mechanical response, UNIVERSALITY OF SENSITIVENESS IN PLANTS 37 and also the normal negative electrical response. We have already seen, in fig. 17, an instance of this abnormal positive followed by the normal negative mechanical response, in experiments with Azophytum. This abnormal positive re- sponse, being a matter of the intensity of the blow delivered by the water-movement, can only exhibit itself under favour- able conditions. It is thus possible in mechanical response to have either the normal response preceded by the abnormal, or the normal response alone. But whenever we have an abnormal mechanical response, due to positive turgidity- variation, we have also simultaneously an _ ab- normal positive electrical response. In fig. 27 is shown a simultaneous re- cord of the two, in which there is a preliminary ab- normal positive mechani- cal response, and a syn- chronous positive elec- trical response, followed Fic. 27. The Abnormal Positive preceding in both cases by the the Normal Negative in Mechanical and normal responses.! It Electrical Responses in Bzophytum represents the moment of application of should be stated here that stimulus. The upper is the mechanical this positive turgidity- and the lower the electrical record. The ae ts : : records downward indicate erection of the variation, which is re- leaf or’ galvanometric positivity. ferred to as abnormal, is of very great importance, and will be seen in Chapter XXX. to be directly responsible for growth. It will be found ! Tt must be understood that this positive electrical response, being dependent on the excitatory expulsion of water at a distant point, is, in a certain sense, a physiological response. For it is the excitability of that distant point which determines the positive turgidity and concomitant positive electrical variation of the point under examination. The contraction of the excited point gives rise to a hydrostatic disturbance, by which a movement of water is brought about. Such a disturbance, then, will be indifferently designated as the hydrostatic, or hydraulic, wave. 38 PLANT RESPONSE helpful in the future if we uniformly distinguish (1) the true excitatory or normal as the drect, and (2) this positive or abnormal as the zvdzrect effects of stimulation. Discrimination of differences of excitability by electric test.— Not only does the electrical response enable us to detect the state of excitation, but I have been able further to devise an electrical test by which differences in the natural excita- bility of two points might be distinguished. The demonstra- tion of the existence of two electrical responses, one of which alone, namely the negative, constitutes the true excitatory effect, is of much theoretical interest. For the hydraulic, or positive electrical effect, has been mistaken for the true excitatory response in plants. As the excitatory effect in animal tissues, moreover, was known to be negative, this fact was supposed to indicate a difference between the proto- piasmic reactions of animal and vegetable. But the experi- ments which I have just described conclusively prove that such a difference does not exist, the sign of response in animal and vegetable being the same. They offer us an explanation, further, of the source of error. A more detailed account of this subject will be found in my forthcoming work on the Electro-Physiology of Plants. Many of the motile phenomena of mechanical response which we shall have to study in the course of the present work are modified by differences of excitability at different parts of a tissue. In the case of the primary pulvinus of Mimosa for example, we can see how the responsive fall is brought about by the evidently greater excitability of the lower half of the pulvinus. But in organs which apparently exhibit little motility, it is impossible from inspection to know whether all parts of the tissue are equally excitable, and, if not, which parts exhibit the greater excitability. Such variation of excitability is often due to invisible molecular differentiation, and eludes visual scrutiny. Fortunately, as already said, I have been successful in devising a mode of electrical investigation by which this differentiation is detected with the greatest certainty. This method will be UNIVERSALITY OF .SENSITIVENESS IN PLANTS 39 found fully described elsewhere, but the results may be summarised as follows: On simultaneous excitation of two points, the current of response flows in the tissue from the more excitable point A to the less excitable point B ; con- versely, if the direction of the responsive current is from A to B, the point A may be taken as the more excitable. By means of the unfailing discrimination of the differences of excitability in a tissue which this method renders possible, it will be shown in the course of the present work that many ot the anomalies of growth-curvature receive a most complete and satisfactory explanation. Fic. 28. Response of Selenium to the Stimulus of Light (Conductivity variation method.) The electrical response given by plant-tissues in general, as described in this chapter, is obtained by means of the difference of electrical potential or electromotive variation, induced as between the excited and unexcited portions of the tissue. There is another method of detection by means of those changes of electrical conductivity which are concomi- tant to excitation in the substance under experiment.' It should be borne in mind that the various responses, obtained by the mechanical, by the electromotive, or by the conduc- tivity-variation method, are merely different expressions of ' Bose, Molecular Changes produced in Matter by Electric Waves, Roy. Soc. Proc. 1 oo. 40 PLANT RESPONSE that fundamental molecular change which underlies excita- tion, and which disappears on the restoration of molecular equilibrium. Universality of responsiveness in matter.—If we take a plant-tissue and subject it to a sufficient degree of cold, its responsive power will be found to disappear. It reappears, however, on the return of the tissue to the normal tempera- ture. The power of response is thus seen to depend on the molecular condition of the substance. Now this irritability, or power of responding to stimulus, may be vaguely regarded as a characteristic property of living substances ; and we may evade the difficulty of any attempt at a real ex- planation by describing it as a ‘vital’ phenomenon. But if we regard all such phenomena as due_ ulti- mately to physico-chemical actions, we cannot rest satisfied with what is, after all, a ..mere. -descriative phrase. Progress can only be made in scientific in- ee ake er itn ome wuty by ate Sega ally to discard all such The record to the left is the normal assumptions of the working response, and the line to the right of mystical forces in favour shows abolition of response on ; ; application. of simpler and more rational explanations. By following the electrical method of inquiry which has just been described, I have been able to prove that the power of responding to stimulus, and, under certain conditions, the arrest of this power, is the characteristic not of organic matter only, but of all matter, both organic and inorganic ;' and that in general the various agencies which bring on the modifica- ! Bose, Response in the Living and Non-Living. UNIVERSALITY OF SENSITIVENESS IN PLANTS 41 tion of response in one case—such as: fatigue, temperature changes, stimulating or depressing chemical reagents—act in the same way in the other. The capability of responding, so long regarded as the peculiar characteristic of the organic, is also found in the inorganic, and seems to depend in all cases, both qualitatively and quantitatively, on the condition of molecular mobility. In the course of the present work, then, the term ‘ physio- logical’ is to be understood as a convenient expression for the phenomena of plant or animal tissues under in- vestigation, and not as in any sense opposed to the word ‘ physical.’ SUMMARY Stimulus causes molecular derangement in matter. The conditions of molecular upset and return to the state of equi- librium correspond to the state of excitation and recovery from that state. The molecular disturbance is attended by various physico- chemical changes in the properties of the substance, among the most important of which in a living tissue may be men- tioned (1) contraction of the excited cell, and expulsion of water; (2) electromotive variation; (3) conductivity- variation. The true excitatory change causes negative variation of turgidity, with depression or negative mechanical response of the leaf, and galvanometric negativity. The intensity of these effects varies with the physiological condition, being totally abolished by those molecular changes which are con- comitant with the death of the tissue. By means of electrical response it is found that, not sensitive plants alone, but every plant and every organ of the plant is excitable. Positive turgidity-variation, by whatever means produced, causes positive mechanical response—or, in the case of Mimosa, erection of the leaf—and galvanometric positivity. 42 PLANT RESPONSE A pulse of positive turgidity-variation is often propagated in consequence of the sudden expulsion of water from a stimu- lated point at a distance. The laws of true electric response to excitation may be summarised as follows: A. The indirect effect of stimulation is a positive electrical variation, indicating a positive turgidity-variation. b. The direct effect of stimulation causes negative turgidity- variation, concomitant with a negative electrical variation. (1) The direction of this responsive current in plant- tissues, as in animal, is from the more to the less excited portions of the tissue; or, the excited point is galvanometrically negative. (2) On simultaneous excitation of two points, the current of response in the tissue is from the more to the less excitable. (3) Conversely, if the responsive current be from A to B, the point A may be taken as the more excitable. CHAPTER TV ON CONDITIONS FAVOURABLE TO THE CONSPICUOUS EXHIBITION OF MECHANICAL RESPONSE Differences of degree of motile sensibility in sensitive plants so called—Response of anisotropic organ brought about by differential contraction—Production ot response by artificial variation of turgidity—Variation and counter-variation ot turgescence, causing two opposite responsive movements—Differences between hydrostatic and true excitatory effects— Distinction of plants as ordinary and sensitive, arbitrary - Sensitive plants may be excited, yet give no mechanical response— Certain conditions necessary to exhibition of differential response— Balanced action as result of diffuse stimulus on radial organ—Slight differential contraction of pulvinus magnified by long petiolar index. By means of the electric mode of investigation detailed in the preceding chapter, I have shown that all vegetable organs, whether of ordinary or of sensitive plants, are excit- able. Hence the supposed absence of motile indications, in ordinary plants, is not to be considered as due to want of sensitiveness, but to the lack of proper conditions for mechanical movement. What these conditions are will be described presently. But before entering upon such considerations, I first wish to demonstrate that even in the matter of the pulvinar motility itself there is no abrupt division, but a very gradual transition, from those plants in which it is scarcely perceptible, to others which exhibit it in a marked degree. Range of sensitiveness in sensitive plants.—_Three typical instances of plants possessing extremely sensitive, moderately sensitive, and almost insensitive leaflets, are: (1) Mimosa pudica, (2) Biophytum sensitivum, and (3) Phil- anthus urinaria. In Philanthus the small leaves borne on pulvini are arranged on two sides of the long twig or petiole, in a manner somewhat resembling the arrangement of leaflets in Bzophytum. ‘The plant appears on casual inspection to be 44 PLANT RESPONSE wholly insensitive, ordinary mechanical stimulation having no effect on its leaves. But if we apply strong thermal or electrical stimulus to the end of the twig bearing the leaves, then they begin to close very slowly, in serial succession. But whereas with Azophytum the response begins almost instantaneously, the maximum being reached in less than a second, and complete recovery attained in about four minutes, in the leaf of Phzlanthus urinaria the latent period, for moderate stimulus, is as long as three minutes, the maximum reached in not less than forty-five minutes, and complete FIG. 30. Responses of (a) quickly reacting Bzophytum, and (4) sluggish Philanthus urinaria, under moderate and (c) under stronger Stimulation recovery may require from two to three hours (fig. 30). It is seen, then, that even with regard to the so-called sensitive plants, there is a wide range of sensitiveness. In some, a slight shock produces quick reaction, in others a very intense stimulation is necessary to initiate response, and the reaction itself is very sluggish. Anisotropy necessary to lateral response.—We shall now try to understand a little more of the mechanics which cause this responsive curvature. In these pulvinated organs, one thing that is noticeable is that the organ is not isotropic, that is to say, its properties are not the same in all directions. CONDITIONS FAVOURABLE TO MECHANICAL RESPONSE 45 The isotropic condition is seen in the case of radial organs, like cylindrical stems or peduncles. One way of showing this is to try to bend such a stem in all directions, when it will be found that equal forces produce equal bending in any direction. - This, however, is not the case with dorsi-ventral organs, such as the pulvinus. This is more pliable in a vertical than in a lateral plane. The mass of cells which constitute the lower half of the pulvinus, in J/zmosa for example, is larger than that of the upper. The lower half is also the more excitable. The leaf remains in a balanced horizontal position, under the action of two opposing forces, the tensions of the opposite halves of the pulvinus, these tensions being modified by the turgidity of the cells. Pfeffer and Sachs have shown that under stimula- tion there is an expulsion of water from these excitable cells. This may be seen if we watch the cut end of a pulvinus very attentively after the disappearance of the excitation due to cut, and during the application of a new stimulus. The application of this stimulus is followed by a visible escape of water from the cut end. Fic. 44. Ineffective Stimulus made Fic. 45. Additive Effect in Electrical Effective by Repetition Response The line to the left shows that single (a) A single stimulus of 3° vibration pro- stimulus produced no mechanical duced little or no effect, but the same response. The curve to the right stimulus when rapidly superposed shows the effect produced when thirty times produced the large stimulus had been repeated six times. effect (4). Leaf stalk of turnip. find, in Chapter XXIJ., that an exactly similar type of response is afforded by Azophytum, where the minimally effective stimulus suddenly produces maximal effect. Relation between stimulus and response in animal and vegetable.—The second type of contractile response is shown by skeletal muscle. Here, after reaching the threshold of response, increasing stimulus causes increasing response, which, however, tends to reach a limit. Exactly parallel effects are seen in the case of plants, in the longitudinal responses exhibited by different radial organs. The experimental method by which this is demonstrated is as follows: The intensity of thermal stimulus may be appropriately increased, as has been explained, by increasing the value of the heating current, the circuit being always closed for a certain definite time, say one second. The resistance of the heating coil being kept constant, the thermal effect is proportional to the square of the current. If, then, we use currents which increase as the square root of the natural numbers, the successive thermal effects will be increased in arithmetical progression. The currents to be used are previously adjusted, by means of an ammeter, and 96 PLANT RESPONSE suitable external resistances. In this way I have obtained successive responses to thermal! stimuli, which increased in arithmetical progres- sion. And we see from the records in what manner the cor- responding responses undergo an increase (fig. 46). It will be seen from this figure that with a stimulus re- presented by unity, Fic. 46. Mechanical Responses to Stimuli in- the response was I°5 creasing in Arithmetical Progressicn divigion Wie a stimulus twice as great, the corresponding response was slightly greater than twice as much, that is to say, it was 3°5. The intensity of response went on increasing, but with special acceleration when the stimuli were four and _ five. After the stimulus of six there was a tendency for the response to approach a limit. The subjoined curve (fig. 47) shows the relation between the increasing stimuli and the corresponding responses. Another interesting fea- ture of these response-curves is one which has already been Fic. 47. Curve showing Relation be- ; tween Stimulus and Response referred to, the prolongation The abscissa represents the stimulus, of the period of recovery with and ordinate the height of response. i i : increasing stimulus. In the present case, with the stimulus of unit-intensity, the recovery was completed in forty seconds. With an intensity twice as sreat, it required fifty-six seconds for the restoration of equilibrium. And this increase in the period of recovery RELATION BETWEEN STIMULUS AND RESPONSE 97 continued progressively, until, with the stimulus-intensity of eight, the time required for restoration to the original con- dition was as much as 4 minutes 40 seconds, or exactly seven times that necessitated by unit-stimulus. All these peculiarities are observable in the electrical responses of plants, as will be seen from the record in fig. 48. The stimulus applied was vibrational, and was increased in amplitude in successive experiments, from 2°5° to 7°5° to 10° to 12°5°. It will be seen that here also as in the case 23° 5 7% 10” 124 Fic. 48. Increased Electrical Response with Increasing Vibrational Stimuli (Cauliflower-stalk) Vertical line to right = ‘1 volt. Stimuli applied at intervals of three minutes. of the response of skeletal muscle, and the longitudinal response of plants, the amplitude of response increasing with increasing stimulus tends to approach a limit. These curves also show the increase in the period required for complete recovery, but in a manner different from that of fig. 46. In fig. 46 the time-intervals were suitably increased, to allow of complete recovery. Inthe case of the electrical responses, however, stimuli were applied at equal intervals of time throughout. This was enough to bring about complete I 98 PLANT RESPONSE recovery in the case of the first two responses. But after- wards it was not sufficient, so that recovery in the last three instances was more and more incomplete, as seen by the tilting upwards of the base-line of the responses (fig. 47). TABLE SHOWING THE INCREASED E.M. VARIATION PRODUCED BY INCREASING STIMULUS! Angle of vibration. Electromotive response 25° "044 volt. LB 075 7°5° 090 ,, 10’ "FOO sa | 12 aS “TOO! es Tetanus.— Having now observed the effect produced by single stimuli, we shall proceed to study the effects of similar Fic. 49. Genesis of Tetanus in Muscle Record to left shows incomplete tetanus, with moderate frequency of stimulation. Record to right shows tetanus more complete, with greater frequency of stimulation (Brodie). stimuli when superposed. In muscle, we find that when stimuli succeed each other with great rapidity, the effect of the second stimulus becomes superposed on that of the first, which has not had time“to disappear. The result is a fusion of effects, more or less complete. With moderate frequency of stimulation we thus obtain incomplete tetanus, which, with ' Bose, Response in the Living and Non-Living, p. 53- RELATION BETWEEN STIMULUS AND RESPCNSE 99 increasing frequency of stimulation, becomes more and more complete (fig. 49). As regards the mechanical response of plants, I obtained similar tetanic effects with the longitudinal contractions of the pistil of Datura alba (fig. 50). Stimuli were here applied at intervals of ten seconds, which was too short an interval, when compared with the natural period of recovery, lasting about two minutes. Hence we obtained incomplete tetanus. This incomplete tetanus became more complete when the stimulation-frequency was _ increased, ' successive stimuli being now applied at intervals of five seconds. It may be noted here that, in the tetanus both of muscle and of plant, the effects of individual stimuli, when rapidly suc- ceeding, become so merged as to appear Fic. 50. Photographic continuous. Jt is only after the maxi- Beene gee ea : Tetanus in Mechanical mum effect has been attained, that the Response of Plants individual effects of stimuli sometimes (Style of Datura alba) become distinguishable by slight oscillatory movements of the curve. In the case of rhythmic cardiac muscle, how- ever, there is no tetanus; and similarly, as described in Chapter XXVII., we find no tetanus in the rhythmic vegetable tissue of Desmodium. SUMMARY There is a minimal intensity of stimulus necessary to initiate response. A stimulus, singly ineffective, becomes effective on repe- tition. Increasing intensity of stimulus produces increasing response, which, however, tends to approach a limit. The effects of rapidly succeeding stimuli in plant-tissues, as in animal, become fused, individual effects being then in- distinguishable. A maximum contractile effect is then pro- duced depending on the intensity of stimulus. T0O PLANT RESPONSE In all the above respects, we find that the responses of plants in general exhibit the closest parallel to the responses of skeletal muscle in animals. But in the animal a different type of response is exhibited by certain rhythmic tissues like cardiac muscle. The re- sponse here is on the ‘all or none’ principle, and sucha tissue cannot be tetanised. In the parallel instances of rhythmic vegetable tissues, the same characteristics are present ; that is to say, the responses are on the ‘all or none’ principle, and there is no tetanus. PART Tf MODIFICATION OF RESPONSE UNDER VARIOUS CONDITIONS CHAP LER «EX ON THE UNIFORM, FATIGUE, AND STAIRCASE EFFECTS IN RESPONSE Uniform response in plants—Staircase effect—Fatigue due to molecular strain —Fatigue in plant-responses—Periodic fatigde—Fatigue under continuous stimulation —Explanation of anomalous erection of leaf of AZzmosa under con- tinuous stimulation—Conductivity and excitability of tissue diminished through incomplete protoplasmic recovery— Relatively greater fatigue in a motile than conducting organ—Disappearance of the motile excitability earlier than con- _ ductivity—Refractory period—Absence of responsive effect when stimulus falls within refractory period. THE mechanical response of plants is fundamentally due, as we have seen, to those molecular changes which are the result of stimulus. These changes bring about contractions of the excited cells, in consequence of .which water is ex- pelled, and we obtain longitudinal response in radial organs, or lateral movement in dorsi-ventral organs, the latter being simply a special case of differential longitudinal contraction. On the cessation of stimulus the expelled water is reabsorbed, and the organ resumes its original position. In the case, for example, of the leaves of A7zmosa, this position of equilibrium is, approximately speaking, at an angle of 45° above the horizon, and this, for convenience, may be called the erect position. After a period of rest, then, molecular equilibrium being re-established, the protoplasm recovers its original properties, of which excitability is one, and response takes place on stimulation as before. This resumption by the leaf of its original position may thus be taken as a rough indica- tion of the restoration of its original protoplasmic properties. But this is only true in a general way, for there may be cases, as we shall see, in which the apparent return of the leaf =: 104. PLANT RESPONSE to its original position is deceptive, and does not really indi- cate a complete protoplasmic recovery. Uniform responses.—If the motile organ, however, be restored, by an appropriate period of rest, to exactly its original molecular condition, and therefore to its original condition of excitability, it is clear that we ought to be Fic. 51. Uniform Electrical Responses (Radish) able to obtain uniform responses to uniform stimuli. That this is true has been shown to be the case, with regard to the responses of the leaflet of Azophytum, and the longitudinal contractions of various radial organs (figs. 18 and 38). By taking electromotive instead of mechanical responses, I obtained a similar result, of uniform responses to uniform stimuli, from various species of plants (fig. 51). In the case of muscle, also, the responses are found to be uniform, if intervening periods of rest be allowed, sufficient for full recovery (fig. 9). ‘Staircase’ effect.—-It is sometimes FIG. 52. Staircase Ef found that a tissue falls into a sluggish fect in Longitudinal a : ’ : Mechanical Response condition, and successive stimuli, by of Plant (Style of : 7 o l l 5 bilit h th Buachirin increasing molecular mobility, have the effect of gradually enhancing the re- sponses, which are seen to increase in a ‘staircase’ manner. I give here an instance of this effect (fig. 52) in the case of longitudinal response, obtained with a style of Hucharis Lily. UNIFORM, FATIGUE, AND STAIRCASE EFFECTS 105 ‘ Fatigue.—It has been said that, when sufficient time is allowed for protoplasmic recovery, the responses are uniform, but that, if sufficient time be not allowed, molecular recovery will be incomplete, and the tissue will remain in a strained con- dition. Under these circum- stances, it is obvious that there will not be a complete restora- tion of the original protoplasmic excitability, hence successive re- sponses will exhibit a diminution or fatigue. The following record (fig. 53) shows this in the case of longitudinal response. Uni- form stimuli were first applied MM Fic. 53. Fatigue in Longitudinal Mechanical Response of Plant (Style of Datura) the case of the first pair of re- sponses, a sufficient interval for recovery, namely one minute, was allowed. When the period allowed for recovery was reduced to halfa minute, there was rapid fatigue ; the second pair of re- sponses shows this immediate effect ; the third pair of responses are to the tenth and eleventh stimuli. Ir i=) at intervals of one minute, by which time the recovery was complete ; and these responses of twenty divisions are seen to be large and uniform. The | stimuli next applied at intervals of half a minute, and the response at once fell to eleven divi- sions. Now, owing to the effect of cumulative strain, the succeeding responses at this interval underwent were (a) (4) (c) continuous diminution, and had fallen as low as to five divisions at the tenth stimulus. In the next figure (fig. 54), fatigue is shown in the electrical responses of plants under Fic. 54. Fatigue shown in Electrical Re- sponse, when sufficient Time is not allowed for Full Recovery In (a) stimuli were applied at intervals of one minute ; in (6) the intervals were reduced to half a minute ; this caused a diminution of response. In (c) the original rhythm is restored, and the response is found to be en- hanced to nearly its original value (Radish). the same conditions, sufficient time for recovery, that is to say, not being allowed. Similar instances of fatigue are well known in the case of muscle. 106 PLANT RESPONSE Fatigue being principally due to residual strain, it is to be expected that, other things being equal, strain will be more persistent with stronger stimulus, as has been shown in the last chapter. It was there shown also that the period required for recovery from a strong was more protracted than from a moderate stimulus. From this it follows that the stimulation-frequency which will exactly allow for com-. plete recovery, and so give rise to uniform responses, in the case of a moderate stimulus, will not be sufficient for stronger stimulus. Hence, keeping the intervals constant, we may Fic. 55. Alternate Fatigue (a) in Electrical Responses of Petiole of Cauliflower ; (4) in Multiple Electric Responses of Peduncle of Bzo- phytum ; (c) in Multiple Mechanical Responses of Leaflet of Bzo- phytum ; and (ad) in Autonomous Responses of Desmodium obtain uniform responses to moderate, and diminished or fatigue-responses to strong stimulus. There is another curious phenomenon, of alternate or periodic fatigue, which I have often observed in the response of plants. The simplest type of such periodic fatigue is that in which responses wax and wane, in regular alternation. Such alternate fatigue is sometimes seen in the electrical response of plants, the multiple response of Azophytum, and also in the autonomous response of Desmodium (fig. 55). Curiously enough, I have sometimes obtained similar alter- nate responses with the compound strip of ebonite and india- rubber, previously described. There are other cases of response in plants, where the variations are cyclic in type, that UNIFORM, FATIGUE, AND STAIRCASE EFFECTS 107 is to say, they consist of groups of responses, which wax and wane alternately. Fatigue under continuous stimulation.—In connection with the induction of fatigue under long-continued stimu- lation, I shall now anticipate certain results regarding the character of the responses given by matter universally— results to be described in detail in the next chapter. It will there be shown that if we represent a given molecular change, caused by stimulus, as positive, the continuation of stimulus will at first increase that change, until it has attained a maximum, after which, under the still continued action of the same stimulus, there will be a reversal or change to the nega- tive. In a living tissue, then, where the incidence of stimulus causes contraction, we may be prepared to find that the same stimulus, long continued, will bring about a reversal of this effect, or, that is to say, a re- laxation. In pa Vane) such a reversal later, Rapid Fatigue under Con- is illustrated in the correspond- ee Stimulation in (az) Muscle ; 2 - : (6) Leaf-stalk of Celery (Electrical ing phenomenon in contractile Response) Hiuselc, It is there found that, whereas the first effect of stimulus is contraction, the same stimulus, when too long continued, brings about relaxa- tion to the original form. In the electrical response of plants under continuous stimulation, I also find this peculiar fatigue-reversal (fig. 56). I have also obtained similar fatigue-reversals in the longi- tudinal response of radial organs. In fig. 56, (@), a series of tetanic electric shocks was continuously applied during a period of four minutes. The maximum contraction was attained in the course of one minute, after which there was a reversal, or relaxation. Under this condition of fatigue- 108 PLANT RESPONSE reversal, the tissue is incapable of the normal excitatory con- traction, but after a period of rest of about seven minutes it FIG. 57. Fatigue under long-continued Stimulation in the Contractile Response of Plants (a) Stimulation by tetanising electric shocks ; (4) stimulation by rapidly succeeding thermal shocks. Continuous lines represent action during stimulation ; dotted lines represent after-effect (coronal filament of Passiflora, magnification forty times). again gives response, which is at first normal, and then, after reaching a maximum, becomes reversed. The second re- sponse is, however, seen to be smaller than the first. I obtained parallel results under the action of rapidly succeeding thermal shocks (fie. 5.7510): I have already described curious instances of alter- nating fatigue exhibited in successive single responses to single stimuli (fig. 54). Fic. 58. Photographic Record of 44 very curious and interest- Periodic Fatigue under Continuous ing effect of this nature Stimulation in Contractile Response ; ‘ (Filament of Uriclis Lily) occurring under continu- ous electric stimulation, is shown in the accompanying photographic record (fig. 58) of responses given by the filament of Uvzclzs Lily. It will UNIFORM, FATIGUE, AND STAIRCASE EFFECTS 109 there be noticed that the maximum contraction is attained in the course of three minutes, after which there is a fatigue- relaxation, which continues up to the eleventh minute. There then occurs a second, though much feebler, response, after which comes a slow and continuous reversal action. So-called ‘anomalous’ response in Mimosa.—In con- nection with the subject of fatigue, I shall here enter upon the explanation of certain well-known responsive effects in the case of Mzmosa which have hitherto been regarded as anomalous. It is generally found that an erect leaf of this plant is sensitive, that is to say, when stimulated it becomes depressed. In this depressed position it is ap- parently insensitive, hence we are apt to assume that the erect posture is one of sensi- tiveness, depression indicating the reverse. It will be found, however, that if a 17z:osa leaf be continuously stimulated by successive blows or taps, Fic. 59. Photographic Records of in the manner of Pfeffer’s Normal Response of JZmosa to : : Single Stimulus (upper figure), and experiment, the leaf will at to Continuous Stimulation (lower first fall. But, though the (figure) blows be continued, the 1 Hs lter cs the lef is, rected in petiole will, after a ‘time, return to its normal erect position. In this erect posture, however, further blows prove to have no effect upon it, the leaf being now insensitive. I give above a photographic record of this effect in Mimosa (lower record, fig. 59), continuous stimulation in this case having been produced by tetanic electric shocks. It will be noticed that after its responsive fall the leaf returns to the erect position, in spite of the fact that stimulus is 1 te) PLANT RESPONSE being continued. It is important to note that the two records were taken with the same specimen, and in immediate suc- cession to each other. The first, or upper, records the response of the leaf to a single stimulus and its recovery ; the lower gives the response to continuous stimulation. In appearance the two records are singularly alike. But though the leaf at the end of each response occupies the same position, the molecular conditions in the two cases are, as will be shown presently, entirely different, inasmuch as in the first, renewed response was immediately obtained, thus showing its sensitive condition; while in the second, the organ was insensitive, and could give no response until after a period of rest of about ten minutes. The explanation of this apparent anomaly is quite clear from the experiments which have already been described, showing that under continuous stimulation the normal longi- tudinal contraction undergoes reversal and passes into relaxa- tion, as is also the case with continuously excited muscle, the motile response of Wzmosa being only an instance of differential longitudinal contraction. We obtain here also the usual sequence of first, normal contraction, and second, the fatigue-relaxation, or posture of erection. To sum up, then, it is clear that our association a the erect position with sensitiveness is not always correct, for the leaf may assume this posture as the result of fatigue. That its position in this case, however, though outwardly imitating that of sensitiveness, is profoundly different, is at once revealed on application of stimulus. The leaf is now irre- sponsive. But if we allow it a period of rest—of some eight or nine minutes in summer, or double that time in winter— the internal molecular equilibrium is re-established. But of this internal readjustment the leaf gives no visible indication. It remains in the same unchangingly erect position. During the course of the cycle, then, it has passed from the normal erect to the normal depressed, and thence to the abnormal erect position. It next passes, without any outward change, from this abnormal erect to the normal erect position, after UNIFORM, FATIGUE, AND STAIRCASE EFFECTS III a period of repose. And this return of the leaf to its normal condition is testified by its once more responding to stimulus. Fatigue of conductivity and excitability.— It has already been pointed out that the protoplasmic properties of a tissue cannot be restored to their original condition after stimulation, without the intervention of a suitable period for the re- establishment of molecular equilibrium. This fact I shall now demonstrate by additional experiments. The restoration of the normal protoplasmic condition in a tissue may be tested by observing the recovery of some of those properties which are capable of measurement. One of these is its conductivity, measured by determining the speed with which excitation travels through the tissue, the method of which determination will be fully described in Chapter XX. Under normal conditions this velocity is constant. If we excite the tissue, and measure the rate at which the excitation travels, and if we then allow a sufficient interval of rest for complete protoplasmic recovery and again determine this velocity, we shall find that the two are the same. But, if the necessary resting interval be not allowed, recovery being incomplete, there will remain a residual strain. The velocity of transmission of excitation will, under these circumstances, be found to be reduced. Another protoplasmic property -which is capable of measurement is the excitatory contraction seen in motile organs, measurable by the amplitude of response. We have seen that this amplitude is constant under normal conditions, and when sufficient intervening periods of rest are allowed. But diminution of the intervening resting period produces diminution of the amplitude of response. From what has been said it follows that, if the intervening resting periods of a tissue be continuously diminished, there will be a continuously increasing residual strain, and this might be detected by the consequent continuous decrease of conductivity and motile excitability. I have been able to verify this deduction by an experiment on a leaf of Liophytum, the details of which will be found in Chapter XX. rr2 PLANT RESPONSE It will there be seen that, as the resting period was gradually shortened to half a minute, the conductivity underwent di- minution from the normal 1°88 mm. to 1°54 mm. per second, that is to say, by 18 per cent. The reduction of motile excitability was found, however, to be still more marked, the height of response being reduced from the normal thirty- four to one division, that is to say, by as much as 97 per cent. On still further reducing the period of rest, from half a minute to ten seconds, it was found that there was no motile response whatever. The tissue is thus seen to be altogether incapable of response during a certain refractory period. The time- value of this refractory period not only differs in different plants, but also varies with the physiological condition. In Biophytum, normally speaking, it is ten seconds, and in Mimosa about one minute. Earlier abolition of motile excitability than of con- ductivity.—We have seen in the last experiment, that while the conductivity of the petiole was reduced by 18 per cent. the motile excitability of the attached leaflet underwent a diminution of 97 per cent. This shows that motile excitability disappears earlier than conductivity. The reason will be apparent if we consider the difference between the two expressions of protoplasmic excitation. We have, in the conduction of the state of excitation from point to point, a direct expression of the transmission of the molecular change initiated by stimulus. The motile response, however, is a somewhat remote consequence of the series of events which follows on the fundamental molecular change. _ Inter- mediate occurrences are the permeability variation and the contraction, in this case differential, which it produces. The movement of the leaf is a result of all of these, and depends for its complete fulfilment on certain favourable circumstances. In any case, there are mechanical obstacles which have to be overcome in forcing the expelled water through channels of escape. That this must involve some degree of waste of force is partly seen in the fact that all excitations do not produce response, it being necessary that the stimulus should UNIFORM, FATIGUE, AND STAIRCASE EFFECTS IT3 exceed a certain minimum value in order to produce any movement at all. If, again, the escape of water should be resisted, owing to the peculiar condition of the tissue, which has already been described (p. 49), then even a strong stimulus would be unable to bring about movement. Analysis of the different phases in the response of Mimosa under continuous stimulation.—Having now con- sidered in detail some of those changes of protoplasmic pro- perties which are brought about by the action of stimulus, we are enabled to study the effect of long-continued stimulation O° 2"a V6 SB O42" 46° 8: Fic. 60. Ineffectiveness of Stimuli, owing to Increasing Fatigue, in Mimosa In the left-hand figure stimuli were applied at intervals of 3-5 minutes. These evoked feeble responses. In the right-hand figure stimuli were applied at intervals of two minutes. Response now became incon- spicuous. Where stimuli were applied at intervals of one minute no effect was produced. The leaf was refractory. from a somewhat different point of view. We saw that with incomplete recovery, protoplasmic excitability was pro- gressively diminished. In order to demonstrate this, in the case of Mzmosa,1 obtained responses at intervals of 3°5 minutes, the uniform stimulus of condenser discharge being employed. The responses, which had been uniform, when stimulus was applied after complete recovery, at intervals of about eight minutes, were now found to be very much reduced (fig. 60). In a second series of experiments on the same specimen, the intervening periods of rest were still further I II4 PLANT RESPONSE reduced, to two minutes ; and it will be noticed that, owing to increasing incompleteness of recovery, the responses were here reduced to the merest indications of twitches. When the intervening periods were still further shortened to less than one minute, the stimuli fell within the refractory period of the tissue, and produced no indication whatsoever of their effect. As an extreme instance, we have the effect of continuous stimulation, already described on p. 109. To be precise, we must remember that in this gradual abolition of response, under quickening stimulation, we not only see the action of diminished excitability, but also of diminished conductivity. The erection of the leaf of J/zmosa, by the relaxing action of fatigue, may also be assisted by the later contraction of the upper half of the pulvinus. For we have seen that in an anisotropic organ, the less excitable half responds subsequently to the more excitable. The contraction of the upper half of the pulvinus in J/zmosa would produce erection of the leaf, and that this might sometimes happen appears probable from the fact that in the erection of the leaf under continuous stimulation it is occasionally found to be lifted above its normal position. SUMMARY Stimulus, by causing molecular derangement, brings about mechanical response; and by molecular transmission of disturbance from point to point, the excitation is conducted toa distance. Excitatory mechanical response and conduction of excitation are different expressions of the effect of stimulus After a period of rest from the action of stimulus, there is a restoration of molecular equilibrium. The protoplasmic properties of excitability and conductivity are then completely restored. Under such normal conditions responses are uniform. A tissue in a sluggish condition has its molecular mobility increased by the action of successive stimuli. This produces the ‘staircase’ effect, of gradually enhanced responses. When sufficient time is not allowed, there is a residual UNIFORM, FATIGUE, AND STAIRCASE EFFECTS I15 molecular strain. The conductivity and ‘excitability of an organ are thus diminished, and the responses undergo diminution, in consequence of cumulative residual strain. Fatigue is greater in a motile than in a conducting organ. Motile excitability disappears earlier than conductivity. The anomalous erection of the Mimosa leaf is brought about by the combined effects of diminution of conductivity, abolition of excitability, and possibly, in some instances, by the subsequent excitation of the upper half of the pulvinus also. CHAPTER 2 THEORIES CONCERNING DIFFERENT TYPES OF RESPONSE The chemical theory of response—Insufficiency of the theory of assimilation and dissimilation to explain fatigue and staircase effects—Similar responsive effects seen in inorganic substances—Molecular theory—When molecular recovery is complete, responses uniform: when incomplete, fatigue brought about by residual strain—Fatigue under continuous stimulation, in inorganic substance, in plant, and in muscle —Staircase effect brought about by increased molecular mobility : examples seen in inorganic substance, and in living tissues—No sharp line of demarcation in the borderland between physical and chemical phenomena Molecular changes attended by changes of chemical activity—Unequal molecular strain gives rise to a secondary series of chemical actions—Volta- chemical effect and by-products—Supposition that response always dispropor- tionately larger than stimulus, not justified —Existence of three types: (I) response proportionate to stimulus ; (2) response disproportionately greater than stimulus ; (3) response disproportionately less than stimulus—Instances of stimulus partially held latent : staircase and additive effects ; multiple response ; renewed growth. Ir has already been shown, in previous chapters, that the various types of response met with in animal tissues are exactly paralleled, even in detail, in the response of plants ; and numerous further instances of this fact will be met with in the course of the present work. It would thus appear that the theoretical explanation of either class of responses must be applicable to the other also. Existing theories regarding animal response, however, have not yet been found sufficient to meet all the difficulties of the case, and it is probable that the larger data now made available by the inclusion of response in plants, may go far to throw light on certain obscurities which are at present regarded as perplexing. Chemical theory of assimilation and dissimilation.— The theory which is generally accepted at present may be referred to briefly as chemical. According to it, living matter DIFFERENT TYPES OF RESPONSE LIF is maintained in a state of equilibrium by the two opposed chemical processes of building up or assimilation, and break- down or dissimilation. Stimulus causes a down or dissimi- latory change, which is again compensated, during recovery, by the building up, or assimilative change. In the case of uniform responses, the two processes exactly balance each other. But on occasions when the down change is the greater of the two, the potential energy of the system falls below par, for the building-up process cannot then sufficiently repair the chemical depreciation caused by the downward change. Hence occurs diminution of response, or fatigue, which is supposed to be further accentuated by the produc- tion and accumulation of deleterious ‘ fatigue-stuffs.’ The disappearance of fatigue, after a period of rest, is explained by the renovating action of the blood supply, which is also regarded as the means of carrying away the fatigue-stuffs. A serious objection to these explanations, however, lies in the fact that even excised and bloodless muscles exhibit recovery from fatigue, after a period of rest. In isolated vegetable tissues, again, where there is no active circulation of renovating material, the same effect, and its removal after a period of rest, are observed. Thus the difficulties met with in explaining fatigue accord- ing to a purely chemical theory are great enough. But still greater are those which we encounter when we come to deal with the staircase effect—typically shown in cardiac muscle —in which successive responses to uniform stimuli exhibit a gradual enhancement of amplitude. Here the result obtained is in direct opposition to the theory described ; for in this particular case, we have to assume that the same stimulus which is usually supposed to cause a chemical break-down becomes efficient to produce an effect exactly the reverse. It is true that the heart, usually speaking, is ¢harged with blood ; but this particular staircase increment of response, under uniform stimulation, is observed even in the initial twitches of bloodless muscle (fig. 64), and here there can be no question of a supply of renovating blood. 118 PLANT RESPONSE Parallel types of response in living organic, and in inorganic matter.—Such being the difficulties involved in the explanation of a single class of phenomena, on the chemical hypothesis of assimilation and dissimilation, it may be well next to turn our attention to the conclusions suggested by the observation of response in matter generally. And for this purpose it is best to take the responses obtained from inorganic matter in particular, the hypothetical assimilation and dissimilation being in that case out of the question. With regard to the mode of observation, I have already explained how the molecular derangement consequent on stimulus may be studied, either (1) by recording the change of form ; or (2) by recording the variation of conductivity ; or (3) by recording the electromotive variation. As an example in the first place of responsive contraction in inorganic matter, we may select for our investigation the response of india-rubber, under thermal stimulation. In this case, chemical changes, either up or down, are impossible. The second, or conductivity variation method, may be used in the case of metallic powders, the stimulus being that of Hertzian radiation. In this case also chemical action may be excluded, the experimental material being usually placed in naphtha. In the third case, again, where response is obtained by means of the electromotive variation, under mechanical stimulus, the substance used is platinum, the most chemically inactive of metals, electrolytic contacts being made by water.' The possibility of chemical action is thus reduced to a minimum, and the assimilatory change entirely excluded. It will be found, however, that in all these cases of in- organic response, in which substances, physically and chemi- cally widely unlike, are subjected to diverse forms of stimula- tion, and have their responses tested and recorded by absolutely different methods, the results obtained are exactly parallel. All alike, when sufficient intervening periods of rest are 1 For details of these investigations and results, see Bose, Response in the Living and Non-Living. DIFFERENT TYPES OF RESPONSE 119 allowed, give uniform responses to uniform stimuli. And when the period of rest is shortened, all alike exhibit fatigue. From the conditions of experiment it is clear that these effects are physical or molecular. The molecular derangement caused by stimulus is thus gauged by the amplitude of response. Recovery is brought about by the restoration of molecular equilibrium, and for this purpose it has now become evident that the process of assimilation is not essential. When sufficient time, however, is not allowed for recovery, we have a residual molecular strain, and a substance in this strained condition is less responsive, as seen in the diminished height of its response. Fatigue is thus due to molecular strain, and its cumulative effects. But when the fatigued substance is allowed sufficient fasta time for the strain to disappear, its Paes Fic. 61. Fatigue-Reversal subsequent responses exhibit the nor- fey Meee eee Con- mal amplitude. tinuous Stimulation of , ; Hertzian Radiation. It was explained in the last chapter / : : The horizontal line repre- that in the case of J/zmosa, as in that See te edition ot of muscle, a complete reversal of Seu te me Mee response is brought about by extreme variation method. fatigue, under continuous stimulation. The following record shows a similar reversal in Arsenic, under the continuous stimulation of electric radiation (fig. 61). It was only after a sufficient interval of rest that this sub- stance gave renewed normal response. It may be added that these fatigue-reversals, as in the longitudinal response of the Uriclis Lily, are sometimes found to be recurrent. This curve of fatigue-reversal in Arsenic under continuous stimulation was obtained by recording the changes of electric conductivity in the substance. A still more striking analogy with the mechanical records of fatigue in plants and animals is afforded, however, by the automatic record given in fig. 62 of contractile responses inindia-rubber. When this substance is excited by rapidly succeeding thermal shocks, we obtain ANY 120 PLANT RESPONSE first, the normal contractile effect, and secondly, the relaxation due to fatigue, in a manner exactly similar to that which characterises the fatigue-reversals of J/Zzmosa and of skeletal muscles. In the present case, the india-rubber attained its maximum contraction in the course of two minutes, after which there was a continuous relaxation. In this response of india- rubber, and in its fatigue-reversal, we have an analogy with the response of living animal tissues, such as muscle, so close as to compel us to the conclusion that both alike are phenomena of molecular response. A mere contraction of the india- rubber might have been sup- posed to be due to the specific action of heat on that sub- stance. And had this been all, successive thermal shocks would have had the effect of continuously increasing the : Ng re contraction, till a limit was Fic. 62. Automatic Record of Fatigue , in the Contractile Response of India- reached. But if, on the other rubber under Rapidly Succeeding hand, molecular excitability Thermal Shocks : The first effect of stimulus is contrac- be a factor in the process tion, but this passes into relaxation of response, we might then under continued stimulation. The : 2 time-marks represent intervals of €Xpect, under certain condi- half a minute. tions of fatigue, a loss of - molecular excitability. That this is actually the case is shown by the fatigue-reversal, attended with relaxation, which is seen in the figure. A muscle, again, in this condition of fatigue-relaxation, after a period of rest-—during which the molecules have time for recovery from their state of strain—becomes once more excitable without any visible change. We found a like phenomenon occurring in the case of fatigued and relaxed J/zmosa, under continuous stimulation. In that case it was, as we saw, a period of rest of about eight or ten DIFFERENT TYPES OF RESPONSE WAM minutes, which made the fatigued J/¢mosa once more con- tractile. Returning to india-rubber, we find that here also a period of rest of ten minutes enables it to recover its excitability, and once more exhibit its responsive con- traction. From these facts it would appear that in order to explain the phenomenon of response, and its various modifications by fatigue and other factors, we have no option but to regard it as an expression of the molecular responsiveness of matter in general. We next turn to the converse phenomenon of staircase response. Since response is an expression of molecular Fic. 63. Preliminary Staircase In™ Fic. 64. Preliminary crease, followed by Fatigue, in the Staircase Increase, fol- Response of Galena to Hertzian lowed by Fatigue, in Radiation the Response of Style (Conductivity variation method) of Bucharis derangement, it follows that its extent will, other things being equal, depend on the degree of molecular mobility. This being so, it is conceivable that a substance, at first in a sluggish condition, may, by the impact of successive stimuli, have its molecular mobility gradually increased, with a corre- sponding enhancement of its response. In fig. 63 we have an example of this staircase effect, in the responses of Gadena. It is to be noticed that this effect occurs at the beginning of the series of responses, as we should expect. After the attainment of maximum mobility, the phenomenon of over- strain is seen, with its accompanying diminution of response, or fatigue. I have obtained similar results in the longitudinal 122 PLANT RESPONSE response of plants (fig. 64). It need only be mentioned, further, that exactly the same preliminary staircase effect, reaching a maximum, and followed by fatigue, is found in the responses of muscle (fig. 65). The inference that the occurrence of the staircase effect is due to the gradual removal of molecular sluggishness, receives further support from the following experiment on a plant where sluggishness is brought on artificially, and made to disappear gradually. Sluggishness may be induced in a tissue by cooling, this con- dition being made to disappear, by the gradual return of the substance to the ordinary tem- perature ofthe room. Oncarry- ing out such an experiment, Abodbbiddidbid ddd did idd Fic. 66. Staircase Increase in Electrical Response of Fic, 65. Preliminary Staircase, Petiole of Bryophyllum, followed by Fatigue, in the rendered sluggish —_ by Responses of Muscle (Brodie) cooling and recording the successive electrical responses to successive uniform stimuli, I obtained, as I had expected, a marked staircase effect (fig. 66). Merging of physical into chemical phenomena.— I have explained that responsive phenomena are primarily due to the molecular distortion caused by stimulus, that is to say, they are of a physical character. Fatigue has also been shown to be primarily due to residual strain. But it must be borne in mind that in the borderland between physics and chemistry there is no sharp line of demarcation. For ex- ample, yellow phosphorus under the stimulus of light is DIFFERENT TYPES OF RESPONSE 123 converted into the molecular red, or allotropic variety. This molecular change, however, is also attended by a concomi- tant change in the chemical activity, phosphorus in the red condition being less active chemically than in the yellow. Under certain circumstances, further, it is possible to have a secondary series of chemical events following upon a condition of unequal molecular strain. We have seen that a homogeneous living tissue, when unstimulated, is iso-electric. When stimulated, however, an electromotive difference is induced, as between the more and the less acted parts of the tissue. The result is an electrical current attended by chemical changes. As a consequence of such volta-chemical action, when prolonged, by-products (fatigue-stuffs ?) may be accumulated, and these may have a depressing effect on the activity of the tissue. Hence, just as after very prolonged activity of a voltaic element it is necessary to renew the active element and change the electrolyte surcharged with by-products, so, after sustained activity of a living tissue, the process of renewal, or renovation, will be necessary. Thus we see how, upon the fundamental molecular derange- ment, a chain of very various chemical events may follow as its after-effect. And it is only by going in this way to the very root of the phenomenon, that we can avoid the many contradictions with which we are confronted by the chemical theory. I have therefore aimed at demonstrating the universal existence in matter of the property of responsiveness, and by taking the simplest cases and excluding as many com- plicating factors as possible, have attempted to show further hew this power of response is modified by those conditions which occasion fatigue, or its converse, the staircase effect. Having thus cleared the ground, it is possibie to take up cases of greater complexity. Different modes of transformation of stimulus.—It has been assumed that response is brought about by a sudden explosive chemical change, the stimulus acting as if on a trigger, for the release and run-down of potential chemical i24 PLANT RESPONSE energy. This implies that response is disproportionately greater than stimulus, and that the responsive change is attended by an evolution of heat and chemical by-products. It would follow, however, from such rapid depreciation, that fatigue must be the invariable consequence of any series of responses. But it is notorious that in the responses of nerve, not only is there no fatigue, but neither is there any evclution of heat, nor occurrence of chemical change, that can be detected. As the simplest case, we have hitherto considered the substance acted on to be neutral in its character, that is to say, not active in the sense of being able to absorb stimulus and hold it latent. But if we regard the living organism as a machine, three different cases are conceivable. These are: first, that in which the responding substance simply con- verts the energy received as stimulus into response ; secondly, that in which the responding substance possesses a large amount of energy, some of which is set free by the action of the stimulus ; and lastly, that in which the responding sub- stance is capable of absorbing and holding latent, to a greater or less extent, the stimulus which it receives. (1) Response proportionate to stimulus.—The first of these types may be illustrated by a responding circuit which con- tains a magnetic motor—say a galvanometer—translating cur- rent into motion. The source of stimulus may be an external battery periodically closed by a tapping key. The waste of energy by the production of heat may be supposed to be brought down to a minimum in this circuit by using a feeble current and reducing the resistance. Uniform stimuli will now cause uniform responses of the responder. Response will be proportionate to stimulus, and there will be no chemi- cal, and no appreciable thermal, change in the responder. This is a state of things which may be said to approximate closely to. the responsive peculiarities of the nerve. (2) Response disproportionately greater than stimulus: re- sponding substance reduced below par.—As an example of the second type, we have to imagine a responding system which DIFFERENT TYPES OF RESPONSE 125 contains a large amount of energy. We may suppose it to consist of a storage battery and a galvanometer, the circuit being normally incomplete. External stimulus may, by some easily arranged mechanism, close the responding circuit periodically. In this case, the responses might be made dis- proportionately larger than the stimulus. There will then be a progressive run-down of the latent energy of the system, and the responses will show diminution, or true fatigue. The system, at the end of the experiment, will be found to be below par. This case may be paralleled by that of highly excitable tissue which is wasted under excessive and long- continued stimulation. (3) Stmulus wholly or partially absorbed.—The third type of substance we have supposed to be one which is capable of absorbing and holding latent, to a greater or less extent, the stimulus which it receives, and under this we may have several important sub-cases. (a) Staircase and additive effects+-The absorbed stimulus may gradually enhance the molecular mobility, with gradual enhancement of response. This is seen exemplified in the staircase effect. Another example of the same thing is probably to be found in the singly ineffective stimulus which becomes effective on repetition. Evidently in this case, the energy of the first few stimuli is held latent in the tissue and added up until it reaches the threshold of response. (6) Multiple response—We can next see the possibility of a very interesting case of stimulus becoming latent. A spring which is immersed in a viscid fluid may, on receiving a feeble blow, give a single vibrational response. But if the blow be powerful, this single strong stimulus will give rise to a multiple series of vibrational responses. Again, a phos- phorescent substance acted on by light absorbs it, and, on the cessation of incident stimulus, continues to give up the excess of latent energy thus acquired in the form of luminous vibration. A selenium cell again, when acted on by a single strong flash of light, I have found to give what may be regarded as two responses, one strong and the other feeble. 126 PLANT RESPONSE From certain metallic particles, again, when exposed to a single strong flash of electric radiation, I have obtained pulsatory responses. From the consideration of all these cases, I was led to investigate the question whether a plant- tissue, when acted on by a single strong stimulus, could be found to give similar repeated responses. From this I was led to the discovery of Multiple Response in plants, a pheno- menon which I find to be very prevalent, and which I shall describe fully in Chapter XX. (c) Response disproportionately less than stimulus : respond- ing substance raised above par.—It is the last of these sub- cases, however, which we could least easily have foreseen. It is as if here the active expression of the incident stimulus were bifurcated. Returning once more to the mechanical model, we may imagine the responding portion of the system to contain, besides the mechanically responding galvanometer, two plates of lead, immersed in dilute sulphuric acid, by which the energy of the stimulating current is partially stored up. The external stimulating current has now to do two things, first, to cause mechanicai response in the galvano- meter, and secondly, to store up an increasing amount of latent energy in the second part of the responding system. It is evident here that, owing to increasing storage, the mechanical response of the galvanometer may become pro- gressively less. We are, perhaps, too apt to ascribe to ‘fatigue’ all cases of diminution of responses. For in this case we shall have an appearance of fatigue which is not due to the run-down of energy, but to its actual increase, in the responding system. I have been able to discover a parallel case to this in the response of plants, a case, that is to say, in which the response is disproportionately smaller than stimulus, successive stimuli nevertheless causing an increase of the energy of the system. I took for my experiment a straight tendril of Passzflora in which growth bad undergone arrest. It may be pointed out here that a certain tonic condition is necessary to the con- tinuation of growth. This tonic condition is determined, as =< DIFFERENT TYPES OF RESPONSE 1277 will be shown later, by the sum total of energy latent in the tissue. From the curve of response given in fig. 67, it will be seen that at the beginning of the experiment, the length of the tendril being constant, the first part of the record was horizontal, instead of descending, as would have been the case had the tendril been growing. The con- tractile shortening brought about by stimulus is here represented upwards. The stimulus of induction shock was now applied at intervals of one minute, and the record shows that the recovery, instead of stopping short at the level of the horizontal line, has proceeded beneath, thus indicating that stimulus has been effective not only in producing contractile response, but also in afterwards initiating growth! (Cf. fig. 178.) And further, as the stimulus goes on causing not only mechanical response, but also accelerated growth, we see that the successive mechanical re- sponses are undergoing dimi- nution. The application of stimulus ceased at the end of the fourth response, and we Fic. 67. Mechanical Response shown upwards in Tendril of observe active growth proceed- Passifiora in which Growth was : : - originally at Standstill ing from this point as a result ee : Stimulation, besides producing of the energy absorbed from mechanical response, initiated the previous stimuli. We have the opposite movement of growth, as shown by the slope thus demonstrated the very of the base-line downwards. curious case in which the build- ing up process is attended by an actual diminution of re- sponse, and where, after stimulation, the energy of the responding organ, instead of being reduced, is raised above par. This experiment explains why an athlete in constant practice does not waste away, but actually increases, muscle. 128 PLANT RESPONSE SUMMARY The principal types of response seen in animal tissues are found also in the responses of plants and of inorganic substances. Three types of responses are possible: (1) that in which response is proportionate to stimulus; (2) that in which response is disproportionately greater than stimulus ; and (3) that in which all or part of the stimulus is, for a longer or shorter time, absorbed by the tissue and held !atent. The subsequent effect of stimulus which is held latent may sometimes be seen in singly ineffective stimulus which becomes effective on repetition ; or in the staircase response, consequent on the enhancing of molecular mobility by the partial absorption of previous stimulus. The fact that stimulus may be held latent for a time, and subsequently find expression, is strikingly shown in the occurrence of multiple response, in answer to a single strong stimulus. It is also possible for the incident stimulus to become divided in its expression, part of it finding an outlet directly in mechanical response, and part becoming latent, and causing accelerated growth. The gradual augmentation of the latter of these may cause a corresponding diminution of the former. An appearance of fatigue is thus brought about, which is misleading, as the decrease of mechanical response is in this case due. not toa diminution, but to an increase of latent energy in the responding substance, as shown by its capacity for renewed growth. CHAR EE ROX I EFFECT OF ANASTHETICS, POISONS AND OTHER CHEMICAL REAGENTS ON LONGITUDINAL RESPONSE Response modified by physiological change--Carbonic acid causes depression, and transitory exaltation as after-effect—Gradual abolition of response in hydrogen and restoration by access of air—Chemical agents cause contraction or relaxation of plant-tissue—Effect of alcohol causing temporary ex.iltation of response followed by depression and protracted period of recovery—Ether causes relaxation and temporary depression of response—Explanation of anomalous action of ether on stimulated A/¢mosa leaf—Abolition of response by hydrochloric acid—Response restored by timely application of ammonia— Abolition of response by poisonous reagent—Similarity of effect of chemical agents on the response of animal and vegetable tissues. THE subject of our inquiry, in this and succeeding chap- ters, will be the determination of those influences which bring about the variation of conductivity and excitability in plant-tissues. The variation of excitability induced in a tissue by chemical reagents, may be studied through the consequent modifications of the responses. Various forms of response which may be used for this investigation are (1) electrical, (2) autonomous, (3) growth, and (4) the simple mechanical response about to be described. A description of the modifications induced in electrical response under the action of chemical reagents will be found on pp. 73-80 of my book, ‘ Response in the Living and Non-Living.’ The effects of chemical reagents on autonomous pulsation and on growth are given in detail in Chapters XXV, XXVII, and XXXV of. the present work. But it may be said here, that the results obtained by all these different methods are concordant, and that the simp'e method of mechanical response now to be detailed is to be regarded as an additional corroboration. It should also be borne in mind that the K 130 PLANT RESPONSE effects of chemical reagents are subject to modification, as is fully explained later (pp. 322 and 477), by the tonic con- dition of the specimen. The method of procedure consists in first obtaining a series of responses to uniform stimuli under normal con- ditions, and then a similar series after the application of the reagent. The uniform individual stimuli in both series are applied at intervals which allow of complete recovery. The intensity of stimulus, and the time-intervals, are kept constant throughout the experiment. The chemical agent may take the form of gas or vapour, or it may consist of a liquid. In the former case, by the turning of the three-way stop-cock described on p. 72, water-vapour is passed through the plant chamber, and the responses thus obtained are taken as the normal. By a quick manipulation of the stop-cock, the gaseous or vaporous reagent is next introduced, and the responses now obtained exhibit the physiological modifica- tion induced by it. The effect produced by some agents is permanent, by others transitory. This difference may be demonstrated by turning the three-way cock once more, so as to allow water-vapour to replace the specific gaseous medium. The responses now obtained exhibit the after-effect. The difficulty in this investigation lies in the selection of specimens which exhibit complete recovery, together with uniformity of successive responses. For this purpose we may take any radial organ, one of the most excitable, and therefore suitable, of these being the filament of the corona of Passiflora. In this case high magnification is not necessary, and very moderate stimulus is sufficient. Failing Passzflora, I have frequently used with success the staminal filaments of the Uriclis Lily, and Brownea ariza, and the styles of Datura and Eucharis Lily. It should be borne in mind that the excitability of the tissue is to a certain extent influenced by seasonal conditions, being different under different circum- stances of time and weather. It is best to choose specimens from flowers which are already open, and in which growth has just ceased. Although, CHEMICAL REAGENTS ON LONGITUDINAL RESPONSE 131 as already stated, this is not the most excitable period for the tissue, yet it affords us the advantage of simplified conditions, inasmuch as, owing to cessation of growth, the line of record before stimulation, or the base-line, now remains horizontal. Stimulation, producing contraction, is represented by up movements of response, and the recovery brings the curve back to the base-line. Effect of carbonic acid gas.—I shall now proceed to describe a few typical experiments, on the modification of response by chemical reagents, out of a large number, which were performed on the radial organs of plants of various kinds, in the course of the season. And first we shall take the effect of carbonic acid. The effect of this gas, after the lapse of about half an hour, is one of considerable depression. By this time the responses are reduced to about half their normal value, and this depression, though slow, is continuous. Fic. 68. Effect of Carbonic Acid Gas This may be looked for as on Longitudinal Contractile Response the general effect, after a (a) Normal response ; (4) after exposure i - to carbonic acid ; (c) transient exalta- certain length of time, of tion after readmission of air. the action of carbonic acid gas. The immediate result of the sudden introduction of an abnormal factor may, however, be slightly different in different cases, according to the tonic condition of the tissue. This immediate effect is sometimes one of brief depression followed by equally brief exaltation, to be succeeded by the true depression. Or there may be a short exaltation, followed by the true depression. The restoration of the normal con- dition, however, is generally followed in the case of carbonic acid gas by a gradually increasing exaltation of the response, which may culminate in double its ordinary height, and after this it again attains the normal (fig. 68). Effect of hydrogen gas.—We next investigate the K2 132 effect of hydrogen gas. PLANT RESPONSE The characteristic effect of depres- sion occurs in this case after a much longer interval than is required by carbonic acid. The immediate result - of application is very erratic and various. There may some- times be an exaltation, or even the contrary, a reversal, of Fic. 69. Effect of Hydrogen Gas (a) Normal response ; (4) after twelve hours’ exposure to H. ; (c) slow revival of response after readmission of air, the normal response. the action of this gas the responses of the tissue are so much diminished as to approach very near abolition. On now, however, allowing air charged with water-vapour to displace the hydrogen gas, the ‘responses undergo a steady revival Fic. 70. Photographic Re- cord showing Effect of Carbon Disulphide in Abolishing Response Normal response, left, abolished seen to by intro- duction of vapour of CS,. with greater rapidity. But after a twelve hours’ exposure to (fig. 69). There is here no sudden exaltation, such as is the general after- effect of carbonic acid gas. Effect of carbon disulphide.— We have seen that the depressing effect of hydrogen takes place very slowly. This is owing to the fact that this gas acts here rather as an agent for cutting off that supply of oxygen that is necessary to the maintenance of the normal life of the plant, than as a direct poison. But we have other gases which are actively toxic, and in such cases the diminution or abolition of response takes place Such an agent may be found in the vapour of carbon disulphide (fig. 70). CHEMICAL REAGENTS ON LONGITUDINAL RESPONSE 133 I may here draw attention to the great advantage offered by the study of the variation of longitudinal response, in determining the nature of the action of various chemical agents. The modifications which these agents produce in the lateral response of the pulvini of sensitive plants are not so simple, inasmuch as we have to deal in these cases with differential action. In radial organs, on the other hand, the response-record gives us indications of the specific action of each modifying agent. In the response itself there are, it must be remembered, two factors which have to be distinguished, namely, contraction in response to stimulus, and the power of recovery from contraction, or relaxation. The diminution and final abolition of response may be brought about, then, in two different ways. The effect of a given agent may be to diminish the normal relaxation which brings on recovery. Successive stimuli will in that case produce a cumulative residual contraction, which places the tissue in a state of strain, in consequence of which subsequent responses become enfeebled. We may, on the other hand, have an agent whose effect is to produce abnormal relaxation. The contractile impulse due to stimulus is in this case opposed by the abnormal relaxation induced by the agent, and we have, in this case also, an enfeeblement and abolition of response. The comparison of the time-relations of the normal and modified curves, together with the trend of the base-line up or down, will show the nature of the reaction, in an unmis- takable manner. All these facts are clearly demonstrated in the experiments and curves given below. — Effect of alcohol.—I shall next describe the action of the vapour of alcohol. Generally speaking, the immediate effect in this case is one of exaltation, though individual idiosyncrasies may sometimes be present, which cause depression from the very beginning. The general effect of this reagent, however, appears to be a prolongation of the period of recovery. So what is gained by brief exaltation is lost again by induced sluggishness. Thus, from the result 134 PLANT RESPONSE recorded on a fast-moving drum, using the style of Datura alba, | find that the height of the normal response was eleven divisions, and complete recovery took place in one minute and a quarter. During the first period of exaltation, after the application of alcohol, the height of the response was increased to sixteen divisions, that is, practically half as much again. But the period of recovery was protracted to four and a half minutes, or nearly three times the period of normal recovery. These considerations will fully explain the series of responses under the continued action of alcohol- vapour, given in fig. 71, where (a2) shows normal response, (6) the immediate and transitory exaltation, and (c)—which Fic. 71. Effect of Vapour of Alcohol (z) Normal response ; (4) immediate temporary exaltation on introduction of alcohol ; (c) subsequent depression. was taken after fifteen minutes’ further application—the diminished responses in which the contraction remainders are a marked feature. On blowing off the alcohol-vapour, however, and substituting fresh air, the tissue is found to recover slowly its normal excitability. If, instead of alcohol- vapour, dilute alcoholic solution be applied, the depressing effect is immediate and very great. Effect of ether.—We now pass on to the question of the action of the anesthetic agent, ether. This produces a relaxa- tion so great as to be incapable of proper representations within the limits of the diagram (fig. 72), where it is merely indicated by the dotted line. It is to be remembered that contraction is shown by lines upward, and recovery, or CHEMICAL REAGENTS ON LONGITUDINAL RESPONSE 135 relaxation, by lines downward. Owing to the predominance of this relaxing tendency, it will be seen that true contrac- tile movement is here very much diminished. Even after the relaxation has attained its maximum, the responses remain insignificant. When the speci- men is not too long etherised, the blowing in of fresh air brings on gradual restoration. It may be mentioned here that ether also produces relaxation of animal tissue. Explanation of anomalous effect of ether on Mimosa.— This experiment on the effect of ether affords a very satis- Fic. 72. Effect of Ether Arrow marks moment of application. This produced relaxation and factory explanation of a phe- depression of response. Air ; . substituted at x, and there is nomenon in the recovery of subsequent recovery of response. Mimosa, which has hitherto been regarded as anomalous. The outspread position of the leaflets is generally regarded as one of sensitiveness ; but, when they have just closed, in consequence of stimulation, if they be subjected to ether-vapour, they open out. Though now, however, mimicking the appearance of sensitiveness, they are in fact over-relaxed, a condition which is one of relative insensitiveness. The explanation may be gathered from the record in fig. 72. It is necessary here to give specific meanings to certain terms which have been used somewhat indefinitely. We know that stimulation causes the fall of the leaf, by differential contraction, and that the organ recovers, or ‘relaxes,’ into its original form after a period of rest. This term ‘relaxation, then, may be properly used as the converse of ‘contraction.’ But, in consequence of the expulsion of water from the organ after stimulation, it becomes flaccid, and this condition also is sometimes vaguely described as ‘relaxed.’ In my own use of the word, however, I shall confine myself to denoting by it that process which is the 136 PLANT RESPONSE opposite of contraction, and which therefore brings about a position contrary to that effected by stimulus. We have seen that ether produces a relaxing effect which is more rapid than the process of relaxation that brings about recovery. And we have seen how, in consequence of this excessive relaxation, excitatory contraction is diminished or abolished. Hence, we see how a stimulated J/zmosa leaflet under ether relaxes into an outspread position, which never- theless is indicative of no re- newed sensitiveness such as ac- companies true recovery. Effect of vapour of hydro- chloric acid.—I shall now deal with the case of _ strongly poisonous agents, of which hydrochloric acid may be taken as typical. On passing the vapour of hydrochloric acid into the plant chamber there was produced a great relaxation, Fic. 73. Effect of HCl Vapour nd the responses underwent a Arrow indicates moment of ap- rapid diminution which ended plication. Depressing effect jn abolition. The effect of this neutralised by antagonistic y : : action of NH, at x. poison is so persistent that the blowing-in of fresh air did nothing to revive the response. But the timely application of vapour of ammonia is found to act as an antidote, restoring the response (fig. 73). Effect of chlorine gas.—This gas also produces a marked depression of excitability, which, under long-continued action, brings about the permanent abolition of response. The accompanying photographic record (fig. 74) shows the effect very clearly. The normal responses to the left are seen to be very rapidly diminished after the application of this gas, the response being reduced to one-eighth of its original value in the course of nine minutes. There are other important considerations in connection with this question, of relaxation CHEMICAL REAGENTS ON LONGITUDINAL RESPONSE 137 or contraction as the direct effects of chemical agents, which it would be out of place to treat in detail here. It need only be stated that these effects, which can be very accurately and continuously recorded by the arrangement of the Optic Lever, are very suggestive. They are found to be modified by the tonic condition of the tissue, the strength of the agent, and the duration of application. Thus, an effect of relaxation may, after a time, pass into the opposite, of contraction. And since these relaxations or contractions of the tissue have a modifying influence on the response, much light on the obscure subject of the effect of drugs becomes possible through this study. I have been able already to obtain several curious and interest- ing results, of which I ey Photographic record, Showing normal here refer to one, in which effect to the left, depressed and Eames drugs, phere on luhich eaeae ot after introduction when applied singly would abolish response, and produce death, are made, when applied in succession, to act as antidotes to each other. It is my intention to show in the course of this book that all the physiological phenomena of the animal have the closest correspondence with similar phenomena in the plant, and this being so, an investigation carried out on the lines indicated, with plants, is likely to be of very great import- ance, practically as well as theoretically. Fic. 74. Action of Chlorine SUMMARY The responsive contractions of an organ afford a reliable indication of the excitability of the tissue. The physiological changes induced in plant-tissues by the action of chemical reagents are outwardly manifested» by modification of response. Certain agents, producing great relaxation, reduce the 138 PLANT RESPONSE power of responsive contraction. Others produce the opposite. effect, thus protracting the natural period of recovery. Hydrogen gas produces a gradual diminution of response, which is restored to its original value on the readmission of air. Carbonic acid causes depression, but the restoration to normal conditions is generally followed by temporary exalta- tion above the normal. Vapour of alcohol causes gradual, and solution of alcohol rapid, depression. This action may be preceded by tem- porary exaltation. The recovery-period is very much pro- tracted. As in the animal, so also in the plant-tissue, ether causes marked relaxation. The depression of response increases progressively with the exposure ; on blowing off the vapour, response is not only restored but may even show an exalta- tion. The opening of the A/zmosa leaflets under ether is not indicative of true recovery but of over-relaxation. A poisonous reagent, like hydrochloric acid or chlorine, permanently abolishes the response. Reagents which individually abolish response may, by their antagonistic character, act as antidotes to one another. CHUAP AIRE Ree Xb] EFFECT OF TEMPERATURE Temperatures optimum, maximum, and minimum—Diminution of electrical response by cooling—-Temporary or permanent abolition of response due to cold—Characteristic differences exhibited by different species—Mechanical response of Bzophytwm and autonomous response of Desmodium arrested by cold—Prolongation of latent period—Diminution of longitudinal mechanical response by cold—Diminution of electrical response of plants by rise of temperature—Similar diminution seen in longitudinal mechanical response— Increase of excitability due to cyclic variation of temperature. ONE of the factors which modify response in plants is temperature. It is known in a general way that certain temperatures are favourable, and others unfavourable, to physiological activity. It is generally understood further that there is a certain optimum, in the case of each species, above or below which the excitability of the plant undergoes diminution. After this, on reaching a certain maximum or minimum temperature, as the case may be, excitability is abolished, and if these unfavourable conditions be long main- tained the plant is apt to be killed. But the problem of the precise determination of such points has hitherto offered insuperable difficulties. Effects of cold: (a) Déiminution or abolition of electrical response.— Already, however, by adopting the electrical mode of investigation, I had been able to overcome these difficulties ; for I had found that the amplitude of the electrical responses, under different temperature-conditions, afforded a means of measuring the excitability of a tissue at the respective points. And now, by the use of mechanical response, I am enabled again to investigate the same problem by new and indepen- dent means. From the results obtained it will be seen that 140 PLANT RESPONSE each method furnishes a remarkable corroboration of the other. I shall now proceed to describe these various results. As an effect of low temperature I have found, by the use of the electrical method, that response undergoes a very great diminution. For example, the subjecting of a petiole of Eucharis Lily to a temperature of —2° C. almost abolished its excitability (fig. 75). (a) Ls When the — specimen, however, was restored to the normal temperature the original response reappeared, and some- times with even greater amplitude than at first. When the plant is maintained at a very low temperature for a con- siderable length of time, the normal electrical response disappears al- together, and the spe- cimen undergoes per- manent death. In this respect, different species (b) of plants have charac- rae ae teristic powers of resist- Fic. 75. Diminution of Response in Zwcharis ance. For example, the Lily by Lowering of Temperature tropical plant, Eucharis (z) Normal response at 17° C. (2) The response almost disappears when plant Lily, after an exposure is subjected to —2° C. for fifteen minutes. of twenty-four hours to (c) Revival of response on warming to 20° C. es temperatuie of 0° ec on being subsequently restored to its normal temperature, gives no sign of revival by response ; whereas the hardier Holly and Ivy, when subjected to the same treatment, do exhibit signs of renewed life (fig. 76). (6) Prolongation of latent period, or abolition of lateral and autonomous responses—Turning now to mechanical EFFECT OF TEMPERATURE I4I response, I find in the case of the plant Lzophytum that the effect of too great cold, or of long-continued exposure to low temperature, is the abolition of its lateral response. But when the temperature is, relatively speaking, only slightly lowered, induced sluggishness is shown in a very interesting manner. Whereas, at the normal temperature, say 23° C., the response of the leaflet of Bzophytum takes place imme- diately, after moderate cooling, on the contrary, the latent a Wet he Ivy Holly Eucharis Fic. 76. After-effect of Cold on Ivy, Holly, and Eucharis (a) Normal response ; (?) response after subjection to freezing temperature for twenty-four hours. period is increased, and response does not begin to take place until from one to two seconds after the application of stimulus. Lowering of temperature also abolishes the autonomous response of Desmodium. (c) Dimzenution of longitudinal response—With regard to longitudinal response, I have been able to demonstrate the effect of cold, by taking a specimen of the coronal filament of Passzfiora. The specimen was kept for fifteen minutes in ice, after which electrical stimulus was applied with no im- mediate contractile effect. Control specimens, on the other hand, exhibited considerable contraction. These contractions were measured in both cases by means of a micrometer. The specimens taken for experiment were all 21 mm. in length. The average contraction of the control specimens was I'5 mm. The cooled specimen, as said before, exhibited no immediate response. But on continuing stimulation for two minutes, contraction began to take place slowly, reaching a maximum of only *5 mm. It must be borne in mind 142 PLANT RESPONSE that the specimen was all this time undergoing gradual warming by the temperature of the room. Another way of demonstrating the effect of cold on the longitudinal response of this corona, is to take three filaments ; of these (a2) has been subjected to cold, (4) is normal, and (c) has been killed by immersion in hot water at 60° C. The right- hand ends of the filaments are so arranged as to lie perfectly even, and a moist cotton thread touches these ends. Fic. 80. Effect of Rising and Falling Temperature on the Electrical Response of Scotch Kale (stimulus constant) in the course of my investigations on the effect of temperature by means of electrical response. This was,a marked increase L 146 PLANT RESPONSE of sensitiveness, which often appears as the after-effect of a preceding cyclic variation of temperature. That is to say, if we take a series of responses while the temperature is rising, and afterwards a similar series while the temperature is falling, it is found that during the process of cooling, the responses are markedly enhanced in amplitude, as compared with those given at the corresponding temperatures during heating. This is seen in a very clear manner in fig. 80. (2) On longitudinal mechanical response-—\1 have ob- served a very similar phenomenon in the longitudinal mechanical response of, for example, the coronal filaments of Passifiora. An ascending series of responses was taken at temperatures of 25°, 35°, and 45° C. They exhibited a regular decrease, as has already been explained. On now, however, taking records of re- sponses during cooling, it was found "that the response at 35°C. was 50 per acs sad Fall of Toop. cent. greater than it had been when the rature on Longitudinal temperature was ascending (fig. 81). Mechanical Response in : Plant I have already explained that response is found to be abolished when the plant is killed, by raising the temperature above a certain maximum point. The exact determination of this point has hitherto been a matter of great uncertainty. I shall in the next two chapters, however, explain several methods by which this investigation may be carried out with great precision. SUMMARY The electrical and mechanical responses of plants undergo diminution under the influence of cold. The latent period of response is prolonged by lowering of temperature. When the temperature is raised above an optimum, the EFFECT OF TEMPERATURE 147 responses, both electrical and mechanical, undergo a diminu- tion. Prolonged exposure to excessive cold or heat brings on abolition of response. Owing to cold or heat-rigor this abolition may become permanent, indicating the death of the plant. CHAPTER 2cnit ON THE DEATH-SPASM IN PLANTS Difficulty of determining exact moment of death—Various /ost-mortem symptoms afford no immediate indication—Ideal methods for determination of death- point— Realised in four different ways: (a) Determination by electrical method —(4) Determination by spasmodic lateral movement at moment of death— Experiments with J/70sa—Death-contraction a true physiological response— Continuity of fatigue and death—Death-point earlier in young tissues—Com- posite spasmodic movement—(é’) Determination of death-point in tendril of assiflora, by sudden movement of uncurling—(c) Determination of death- point by method of volumetric contraction of hollow organ, causing expulsion of contained water. IT is known that when the temperature to which it is subjected is raised above a certain maximum, a plant is killed. But it is very difficult to determine at what exact temperature this takes place. One reason of the difficulty lies in the fact that hitherto a sure criterion of death, which would give an zmumedzate and reliable indication of its occur- rence, has not been generally available. Its various symptoms, such as drooping, withering, discoloration, and the escape of coloured cell-sap, do not manifest themselves at the moment of death, but at some time indeterminately later. Even when a plant has been subjected to a temperature in excess of the fatal degree, it continues to appear fresh and living, and it is not till after some greater or less interval that the death-symptoms are seen. Various investigators have taken up different indications as the criteria of death, and this fact accounts for discordance in the results, which would already have been sufficiently uncertain even had a common standard been decided upon. Exact methods of determination of death-point.—A good method for the determination of the death-point would THE DEATH-SPASM IN PLANTS 149 consist in watching the waning of a given effect, characteristic of the living condition. Still better would be the discovery of some effect suddenly and strikingly manitested at death. But the ideal method would be found, if some effect could be detected which at the moment of death would undergo sudden reversal to its opposite. In this last case, there would not be even that minor degree of uncertainty which is incidental to the determination of the exact vanishing-point of a waning effect. I have been successful in devising four distinct means, by which the death-point might be detected with precision, and it will be shown that all these different modes of investigation enable results to be obtained which corroborate each other in a remarkable manner. The four means are: (a) the method of electrical response; (0) that method by which the point of death is determined from the occurrence of a spasmodic movement, in a dorsi-ventral or anisotropic organ ; (c) that method which depends on the sudden expulsion of water at the moment of death from a hollow organ, previously filled with liquid; and (d@) the method in which the death-point is determined from the sudden reversal of a thermo-mechanical response-curve. I shall, in the course of the present chapter, describe the first three of these, leaving the fourth method to be treated in the next chapter. (a) Determination of the death-point by electrical response.— As regards the electrical method, I have shown elsewhere ! that the response of norma! galvanometric negativity is cha- racteristic of the living condition of a plant-tissue. When the plant is killed, by any means whatsoever, this normal response disappears. At the moment of death from rise of temperature, therefore, we shall have the abolition of the normal negative excitatory response. But at or beyond this point, on the other hand, we may have the positive response of hydrostatic dis- turbance replacing the true excitatory effect. By this electrical mode of investigation, I have been able to determine the death-points of different plants. In the following table, for ' Bose, Response in the Living and Non-Living, p. 62. 2 (oy oO? 150 PLANT RESPONSE example, the specimens tested were radishes, and the experi- ment was conducted during the winter season in England. It would appear from the results given, that in these six cases response begins to be abolished at temperatures varying from 35° to 55° C. It will be shown later that the death-point depends on the season, being a few degrees lower in winter than in summer. TABLE SHOWING EFFECT OF HIGH TEMPERATURE IN ABOLITION OF RESPONSE AND DEATH OF PLANT Galvanometric re- | Galvanometric re- Teimperature sponse of specimen Temperature sponse of specimen at given temp. at given temp. (a7 CR ees 70 dns (ata Cos) keteereene 8o dns. tae ( (53° Caterer eee ans AT 5 ae (55° C. skacesaaee ieee er {i (GAS RAE cece TOO! wa Ae C, vases acute AO (2 ) ie o Cc (5) (6 x Cc 53 Benoa aesnoceacccasc Te 1 0 © cocice eclelevistaielienme 2 ee fe ( OP second ose TOO i. (7° Cy osctieseoeepemenen Gomes. (3) (6) - ( so” CAR 2 (55° Co cawseacbaec eee OL (6) By observation of the spasmodic movement of lateral response.—I now turn to the second of the four methods which I have named, that in which a spasmodic lateral movement is looked for, in a dorsi-ventral or anisotropic organ, at the moment of death. It has been shown that when an electric shock of moderate intensity is applied to an anisotropic organ, say the leaf of J/zmosa, response occurs, in consequence of molecular derangement, and recovery takes place on restoration of molecular equilibrium. If the shock, however, be excessive, response occurs, it is true, but there is no subsequent recovery, owing to the fact that the molecular derangement has passed beyond the point where restoration was possible. There is thus a per- manent, irreversible ‘set, and the organ is now said to be killed. At death, then, a sudden irreversible molecular derange- is THE DEATH-SPASM IN PLANTS I5l ment is produced. It follows that if we could bring on very gradually those conditions which cause death, then, on arrival at the critical point, we might expect the irreversible mole- cular derangement to occur abruptly. If, further, throughout this process, the organ could be protected from stimulation, we might expect that this sudden molecular derangement would also be attended by a correspondingly sudden evidence of excitation, which would in this case, however, be at once the first indication of excitation and the last sign of life. This spasmodic movement we shall designate as the death- response. As regards the protection of the experimental organ from accidental stimulation, it is to be remembered that excitation under ordinary circumstances depends upon some sudden variation of environmental conditions. A sudden change of temperature may thus act as a stimulating agent and produce depression of the leaf. But a gradual change will not act as a stimulus. The effect of such a gradual variation, on the contrary, as will be shown presently, is to produce no excita- tory contraction whatsoever. If now we take a specimen of Mzmosa and place it suddenly in warm water, say at 35° C.,a responsive collapse of the leaves will at once occur. But if the plant be placed in water at the ordinary temperature of the room, and the tem- perature gradually raised—say at a rate of 1° per one minute and a half, or thereabouts—there will be no responsive downward movement whatsoever. On the contrary, owing to absorption of water by the organ, and also to the re- laxing physiological action of heat, a delicate method of record will show a slight and continuous movement upwards. This proceeds till we reach a degree of temperature which proves to be the death-point. For example, in the case of a particular experiment in summer, with a young leaf of Mimosa, when the temperature of 59° C. was reached, there was a sudden spasmodic movement of the leaf downwards. This was, in fact, the death-throe of the plant. In winter, after a spell of cold weather, when the physiological condition 152 PLANT RESPONSE of the tissue was somewhat depressed, this spasmodic move- ment was found to take place at 53° C., which exactly agrees with that of radish, under similar conditions. That this was the true death-point of the A/7zmosa specimen was proved when, on trying the electrical test, it was found that the normal electrical response had disappeared. If, again, one branch of J/zmosa on the intact plant be bent over, and subjected in the manner described to the death-temperature, we find, on examination after a considerable lapse of time, that whereas the leaves of the rest of the plant are still fresh and healthy, reacting to stimulus, those of this branch may be seen, from their dried and shrivelled condition, to be quite dead. Death-response a true physiological response.—It will be shown that this death-response is a true physiological response. Under normal conditions, it will be found to be extremely definite, even in different plants. But, under physiological modification, it varies appropriately with the season, age, condition of freshness or fatigue, and the action of chemical reagents. I shall first, then, demonstrate the effect of age on the death-point. Thus, on immersing a branch of J/zmosa in water whose temperature is raised con- tinuously, we find that the spasmodic movement of death occurs earlier in the young leaves than in the old. Young seedlings, again, have a lower death-point than mature plants. The following table gives results bearing on this fact. The death-points of different plants in the same season and of the same age are, however, so definite as to be almost like a physical constant. This will appear from the following tabular results, and also from results given in the next chapter. A fact which will be explained later must be stated here. In these experiments with continuously rising temperature, it is found that the first spasmodic movement downwards is succeeded by a later, upwards, by which time the temperature has risen a few degrees. The temperatures of both movements are given in the accompanying table, corresponding to their occurrence in two leaves, one old and THE DEATH-SPASM IN PLANTS 153 one young, of each plant tested. These experiments were carried out at the beginning of spring. TABLE SHOWING DEATH-POINTS IN OLD AND YOUNG LEAVES OF DIFFERENT SPECIMENS OF MIMOSA Specimen Young Leaf-organ Old Leaf-organ | / ‘pies | Temperature | Temperature | ‘Jemperature | Temperature | | | corresponding to | corresponding to | corresponding to | corresponding to fall fall later erection later erection | e of | ay | ile CoS 61°5° | 60°8? 62° 1G 59° | 61°5° 61° | 63° TUL: 60° 62 61°2° | 63:6° | Mean . 5Bo75° Gr 72 Gro™ 62°9° From these results it will be seen that there is a mean difference of 1°5° C. between the death-responses of old and young leaves. It would thus appear that the age of a cell must be the occasion of a certain amount of protoplasmic change, as manifested in the retardation of death-response. We may also infer that sudden change to unfavourable physiological circumstances—before the plant has accommo- dated itself to the changed condition—will tend to lower the death-point. This fact I found illustrated during the preva- lence of an extraordinary wave of cold, which supervened recently, during the progress of these experiments. I then found that the mean death-point, in the case of various plants, was reduced by several degrees. In Mzmosa it fell from the avetage of 59° C. to 55° C,, ze. as much as 4° C. We have thus seen that the physiological differentiation concomitant on protoplasmic change is attended by variation of death- point. Explanation of the subsequent erection.—In animal tissues, the contraction produced by 7zgor mortis is succeeded by a relaxation. The contractile death-spasm in a plant is, similarly, followed by the relaxation seen in the subsequent 154. PLANT RESPONSE erection of the leaf. There is another and very interesting point of view, from which we may see in this phenomenon the continuity of fatigue with death. In the curve of reversal, due to fatigue, in Mzmosa, we saw that the first contraction, induced by strong and long-continued stimulation, passed into subsequent relaxation. In this latter state, the molecular condition was such that responsiveness was abolished. It was as if, in other words, the tissue had passed into a temporary state of death. It is true that, if the stimulation had not been excessive, the organ would recover its sensitiveness, after a period of rest. But this transient would pass imper- ceptibly into the permanent condition of death if, on the other hand, stimulation had been excessive. In that case, after the fatigue-reversal, the tissue would remain permanently irresponsive. I have already said that the death-spasm is an instance of excitatory response to intense stimulation, and we should therefore expect the same kind of effect to be produced as is caused by excessive stimulation, that is to say, a preliminary contraction, followed by relaxation, after which there is no recovery. Again, we saw that in the fatigue-reversal of Mimosa, the subsequent erection of the leaf, mainly due as it was to relaxation, was possibly also aided by the later con- traction of the less excitable upper half of the pulvinus. Similarly, it might be expected that the death-contraction of the less excitable upper, would take place slightly later than that of the lower, half of the pulvinus. We have also seen that the excitability of a tissue declines with age, and this decline would naturally be greater in the more excitable half. Thus the difference of excitability as between the two halves would at the same time tend to disappear. As, then, this spasmodic movement in dorsi-ventral organs is a true instance of differential excitatory response, it would appear that the younger the organ, the greater is the excitatory spasm caused by death, and in experimenting with Mzmosa I have found that at the death-point hardly any spasmodic movement is shown by old leaves. These considerations will also explain THE DEATH-SPASM IN PLANTS 155 why older leaves give less motile indication than the young, in response to stimulus in general. (6') By observation of spasmodic movement of uncurling.— I shall now proceed to demonstrate that the death-move- ment which we have seen in JM/zmosa is in its essentials characteristic of anisotropic response in general; and this may be shown by taking a spiral tendril of Passzflora which has become anisotropic by curling. In order to detect and measure with ease the responsive movements of uncurling and curling the experiment is arranged as follows: a light index is attached to the tip of the spiral, and the whole is immersed co-axially in a glass cylinder filled with water. A strip of paper, marked with degrees, is wrapped round the outside of the cylinder on the plane of the index. The temperature of the water is now raised very gradually, and the responsive excursion of the index is read on the graduated circle formed by the paper. It is to be borne in mind that the true excitatory response of the tendril is given by uncurling, which here corresponds to the fall of the leaf of J/zmosa; thus the movement of erection would be represented by that of curling. In the case of Mimosa we saw that the first effect during rise of temperature (due to absorption of water and relaxation) was slow and gradual erection. On the arrival of the death- point of the organ, however, this preliminary rise was succeeded by a sudden responsive fall. The subsequent re- laxation of death then produced an opposite movement, of erection. Similarly, in the death-response of the spiral tendril of Passzflora, we observe parallel phases. There was first a slow and continuous movement of curling, during the preliminary stages of warming. But this movement ceased when a temperature of 57° C. was reached, and the tendril remained stationary for a time. At 59° C., however, there was produced a sudden excitatory response of death by an uncurling, executed with great rapidity, an angular movement of 360 degrees being described by the index during the course of the next few degrees of rise in temperature. With regard 156 PLANT RESPONSE to the short stationary period, it is to be borne in mind that the death-point depends on the age of the tissue, and in the tendril we have different parts in different stages of growth. Hence while the uncurling movement of death was being initiated in younger portions, older parts of the tendril were still moving in an opposite direction. The outcome of these antagonistic movements was a resultant pause, which only lasted for a little while, and was followed by the vigorous movement of uncurling, caused by the death-contraction of the whole tissue. After the completion of the uncurling movement, there followed the opposite, namely, the move- ment of fost mortem relaxation. In a second experiment with a younger specimen of tendril, I obtained results almost identical. Here the uncurling response of death began at one degree of temperature earlier, namely at 58° C., and the index moved through 150 degrees of the circular scale. On the subject of the death-contraction of the radial organs of ordinary plants, I shall speak in some detail in the next chapter, and shall there describe the perfected apparatus by which the thermo-mechanical response can be continuously recorded, the curve exhibiting the death- point with great precision. (c) By observation of volumetric contraction, causing sudden expulsion of water.—For the present I shall describe only the third of those methods which I have enumerated for the determination of the death-point, that namely which depends on the sudden expulsion of water, at the moment of death, from the hollow organ of an ordinary plant, previously filled with liquid. The specimen used for the present demonstration will be the peduncle of A//zum, although there are many tubular organs of various species of plants which are more or less suitable for these experiments. We cut a length of about 10 cm. from the middle of a peduncle of Adium, rejecting the too young and too old portions at top and bottom. As the presence of air-bubbles is likely to be disturbing to the experiment, the water used must have been previously boiled. The specimen is placed in THE DEATH-SPASM IN PLANTS 157 a vessel of this water, and as a further precaution against air- bubbles clinging to the interior of the tube, the whole may be put inside the receiver of an air-pump and subjected to a repeated partial vacuum. The removal of air-bubbles may also be effected by rinsing the tube of Alum in water containing a small quantity of ether, and immediately after- wards washing with ordinary water. This must be done, however, with caution, as the presence of an appreciable quantity of ether would be likely to affect the excitability of the tissue. Before commencing the experiment, it is advis- able to allow the specimen to remali1 immersed in water for about half an hour, by which time it becomes fully turgid. The lower end of the A//zum tube filled with water is closed by a piece of solid glass rod, and the upper end is also closed : with a piece of glass tube, Fic. 82. Determination of Death- : ‘ point in Adium Tube by Ob- having a capillary bore. : A servation of Volumetric Con- eraduated scale is placed behind traction, causing sudden Expul- : sion of Water this latter, so as to measure the : : A, record given by older specimen ; movement of the water-index, or death-point 63° C. 8, record this movement may be con- of younger specimen; death- f p point 59° C. tinuously recorded on a revolving rum (fig. : e Zum ~P ration 1 w placed ina d He. 150).- The Adz, preparation is now placed vessel of water, and subjected to a gradual rise of temperature in the manner already described. If, at the temperature corresponding to death, there should now be a sudden excitatory contraction of the A/a tissue, the volumetric change thus produced will force out the contained water, and we shall observe a relatively rapid expulsive movement of the water-index. From the curves given above in fig. 82, it will be seen that this occurs at a temperature of 59° C. in a younger, and at 63°C. in an older specimen. Previous to this, there was an inward movement of the water-column corresponding to the gradual 158 PLANT RESPONSE preliminary rise of the J/zosa leaf or curling of the Passzflora tendril; But at the death-point this movement was at first arrested, and then reversed with accelerated speed. The condition of this particular specimen was afterwards tested by the electrical method, when it was found that, while a portion of the peduncle previously cut off gave the normal response of living tissue, the specimen which had been subjected to the death-temperature gave no response. SUMMARY The death-point of a plant, under heat-rigor, is concomi- tant to the disappearance of the true excitatory response of galvanometric negativity. After this point is passed, hydro- static disturbance generally gives rise to the reverse positive response. When an anisotropic organ like the pulvinus of J7/zmosa is cradually raised in temperature, then, at a certain critical temperature, a spasmodic movement is produced which proves to be the death-response of the organ. This is a true excitatory response. The critical death-points of similar specimens are very definite and practically identical. The death-point is modified by the physiological condition of the tissue. Other things being equal, death occurs earlier with a young than with a mature tissue. The death-response of an anisotropic organ is composite. In JMZmosa it consists of a down, followed by an up, move- ment. This is due tc the death-contraction being followed later by the post-mortem relaxation of the organ. In the spiral tendril of Passzflora, the death-response is given at the critical point by a sudden uncurling. In the case of the hollow peduncle of Aum, the death- response is exhibited by volumetric contraction, producing sudden expulsion of contained water. CHAPTER XIV: THE DETERMINATION OF THE CRITICAL POINT OF DEATH BY INVERSION OF THE THERMO-MECHANICAL CURVE Death-spasm in anisotropic organ due to differential longitudinal contraction—In radial organ the death-contraction is purely longitudinal—Death-point deter- mined from point of inversion of a thermo-mechanical curve—The complete record thus constitutes a curve of life-and-death, the two being separated by the death-point—Characteristic thermo-mechanical eurve as resultant of varia- tion of temperature and variation of length—The necessity of specifying the rate of rise of temperature—The thermo-mechanical curve characterised by sharp and definite inversion at point of death—No inversion of thermo- mechanical curve after death of plant—Death-contraction under heat-rigor in plant analogous to similar phenomenon in animal—The J/orograph, a perfected form of apparatus for determining critical point of death—Remarkable identity of thermo-mechanical curves obtained with two similar specimens—Death-point almost as definite as a physical constant—Vanishing of point of inversion with age— Determination of death-point under cold-rigor—Constancy of death-point. WE have seen how the death-point can be determined in anisotropic organs, by the occurrence of a spasmodic lateral movement. We have also seen that this death-movement is an excitatory response, at once initiated and terminated at the point of death. It has been shown further that the anisotropic is simply an instance of differential longitudinal response. It follows that if the death-spasm in the aniso- tropic organ be indeed caused by true excitation, then from a radial organ, at the moment of death, we should obtain a purely longitudinal contraction. We may look upon this phe- nomenon, again, froma purely molecular standpoint. We can then see that if death be brought about by a sudden mole- cular change, such an event might be expected to exhibit itself in a correspondingly sudden change of form. Let us 160 PLANT RESPONSE then take a radial tissue, and subject it to a gradual rise of temperature, taking a continuous record of its variation in length. From what has already been said it will be under- stood that, the variation being gradual, no responsive con- tractile effect will be induced during the process, but, on the contrary, some relaxation. At the death-point, however, a sudden inversion of the curve, due to death-contraction, may be expected to appear, and thus the whole record will consti- tute a curve of life-and-death, this point of inversion separating the two. Should the inversion prove to be very abrupt, the turning-point will afford us a means of determining the temperature at which death occurs, with very great accuracy. In order to prove, further, that this specific response is definitive, we may, after passing the death-point, bring the tissue back to its original temperature once more, and repeat the process. The record ought now to show no inversion characteristic of a transition from life to death. Means of obtaining thermo-mechanical record.—I shall now proceed to describe the manner in which I have obtained this thermo-mechanical record. I took aspecimen of a radial organ, in this case the long style of Datura, and fixed it toa small glass rod which in its turn was fastened to a weight, the whole being placed in a vessel of water. The free end of the style was attached to one arm of the Optic Lever. The bath was now warmed gradually, a thermometer indicating the rise of temperature. Variations of length corresponding to the rise of temperature were progressively recorded, from the movement of the spot of light. For this purpose the mode of procedure was as follows: The vertical movement of the spot of light—occasioned by the variation of length of the specimen—was converted into lateral, by reflection from a second mirror. The paper wrapped about the recording drum was divided into millimetres. It was required that the abscissa of the thermo-mechanical curve should represent temperature, and the ordinate the corresponding length. The position of the spot of light, at any given temperature, was marked on the drum. At each rise of temperature of DETERMINATION OF CRITICAL POINT OF DEATH 161 i° C. the drum was rotated, say, through a distance of 2 mm., and the position of the spot recorded. In this way, by con- necting the recorded points, a curve was obtained, in which the length corresponding to each temperature was known. In this curve an abrupt inversion, due to: sudden death- contraction, was found to occur at about 59° C. The curve thus obtained, however, though the successive points recorded were very near each other, is the result of intermittent observations. Again, two observers were required, one to read the temperature, and the other to take the record. It was therefore subject to error of thermometric reading. Means of obtaining automatic record.—For this reason I was desirous of obtaining a curve which should be con- tinuous and practically automatic, the plant itself being made to record its own variations of length, and its own death-point. The problem resolves itself into that of making the reflected spot of light partake of two motions simultaneously, namely, a horizontal movement proportional to the change of tempera- ture, and a vertical movement proportional to change of length. The horizontal, or thermometric, component of the movement I secured as follows: I constructed a thermo-electric element of iron and nickel, one junction of which was placed in melting ice, and the other junction in the vessel of water containing the specimen whose temperature was being subjected tochange. This element was placed in circuit with a resistance box and a sensitive reflecting galvanometer. The amount of the movement of the galvanometer spot of light could now, by interposing suitable resistance, be brought to any appropriate value. In my experiments, with a particular galvanometer, the movement of this spot of light, for each degree of rise of temperature, was 2°5 mm.—z.e. one-tenth of an inch—when the recording surface was at a distance of 125 cm. from the galvanometer. This extent of movement was quite sufficient for the purposes of the experiment, as it enabled estimates to be made with ease, correct to one-fifth of a degree. By inter- posing smaller resistances, however, one-twentieth of a degree could easily be discriminated. The excursion of the spot of M 162 PLANT RESPONSE light was now found to be strictly proportional to the rise of temperature. 5 In order to combine this horizontal thermometric move- ment with that vertical movement occasioned by the variation in length of the specimen, the vertically moving spot of light from the Optic Lever was thrown on the galvanometer- mirror. The apparatus, it should be mentioned, was so arranged that the two mirrors were as close together as possible. The spot of light now, having been reflected from two mirrors, directly described a curve in which the abscissa gave temperature-variation, and the ordinate, varia- tion of length. When the source of light is a point, that is to say, a pinhole with electric light behind—the excursion of the reflected ray upon a photographic plate will produce an automatic record. Or the movement of the light may be followed continuously with a pen. Conditions for securing accurate death-point.—Here I must point out certain conditions which must be kept in view if we are to obtain a very definite death-point. We know that if a plant be placed in an unfavourable environ- ment, or in a temperature much above the optimum, for a prolonged period, death will ultimately ensue. But inas- much as these temperatures would only cause the death of the plant by indirect and cumulative action through pro- gressive derangement of the several functions, they cannot in themselves be said to constitute death-points. To be scientifically precise in such a determination it is necessary that we should discover a temperature which is of itself efficient to initiate sudden death. On the other hand, again, as the contraction of death is a phenomenon of response, we see that it must have a certain latent period. Some interval elapses, moreover, during which the tissue is attaining the temperature of the bath in which it is placed. Now if the rate of rise of temperature be too rapid, then, owing to the lag caused by these last two factors, by the time the death-response commences, the recorded temperature may have gone beyond the actual death-point. DETERMINATION OF CRITICAL POINT OF DEATH 163 We thus arrive at two conditions which must be regarded as mutually somewhat antagonistic. In the first place, in order to obtain the immediate point of death, it is essential that the plant undergo an exposure which is not too pro- longed. In order, on the other hand, to make due allowance for the latent period and for attainment of the surrounding temperature, the rate of rise must be gradual and definite. In the case of tissues which are not too thick, the latter of these conditions is amply fulfilled by a rate of 1° C. per minute anda half. We see, therefore, that in precise deter- minations of the death-point, the rate of rise must always be specified. With thick stems, however, owing to relative want of thermal conductivity, the attainment of surrounding’ tem- perature and occurrence of death throughout the whole of the organ is a very protracted process. The experiments which I shall describe were made with specimens which were not too thick, death at the fatal point being ensured, when the rate of rise of temperature was that prescribed, namely, 1° C. per every minute and a half. This definite rate of rise may be secured by using an electric heating apparatus, such as is commonly employed for boiling a tea-kettle. The current from the street-mains, which is 220 volts, heats water too rapidly. But the desired rate may be obtained by interposing an electrolytic rheostat of copper sulphate, the current being brought to a suitable value, by separation of the two electrodes through which the current enters and leaves the electrolyte. In this case, when placing the specimen in the experi- mental bath, it is advisable to secure it to a bent glass rod, which rests outside. For if it is placed in the metallic vessel itself, the record will be subject to a certain disturbance, owing to the expansion of the supporting metal while heating. The expansion of the glass rod is so small as to be negligible. In this way, using for experiment a filament of the corona of Passzflora, | obtained a record, showing a very abrupt inversion, corresponding to the death-point, which was M 2 164 PLANT RESPONSE between 59° and 60° C. I shall presently have occasion to describe in detail the various characteristics of this curve. Having described the apparatus with which these curves were recorded, it is necessary to point out the difficulties which were encountered in working with it. It must be . remembered that the excursion of the spot of light, in this case, represented a high magnification of the actual move- ments involved. The spot of light, moreover, was re- flected from two separate instruments, and was liable to be disturbed by the slightest jar or tremor in either of them. Though the instruments were placed on a steady stone pedestal, even this precaution could not be made wholly effective, in the heavy traffic of a town. It was only, there- fore, in intervals of quiet that approximately perfect results could be obtained. This difficulty led me to the devising of a much simpler and more perfect instrument, which I shall designate as the MWorograph.' This is a small and portable apparatus, self-contained, in which the necessity of a galvano- meter is obviated. By its means, moreover, the record is unaffected by any earth-vibration. The Morograph.—-The thermometric record is produced by means of the curling and uncurling of a spiral compound strip, of two metals, having different coefficients of expansion. In order to give strength and steadiness, this helix, which is about 2°5 cm. in diameter, is made of somewhat thick strips of brass and tinned iron, soldered together. By increasing the number of turns in this spiral, the extent of movement per degree in the thermometric record may be increased at will. In my own JMorograph, a helix of three circles was found to answer all requirements. The last half-circle of the lower end of the spiral is fixed to a heavy circular stand of brass, 3 cm. in diameter. The topmost half-circle, on the other hand, has had the tinned-iron strip cut off, and therefore consists of brass alone. It will thus be understood that a line drawn diametrically across this last half-circle ! This word is derived from the Sanskrit root #77, Latin mors, death. DETERMINATION OF CRITICAL POINT OF DEATH 165 would rotate round a vertical axis passing through the centre of the spiral, under the influence of the differential expansion or contraction produced in the compound strip of metal by rise or fall of the temperature. When the outside of the circumference of the spiral consists of the more expansible metal, brass, then a rise of temperature will produce the movement of curling. The difficulty in the construction of this part of the apparatus lies in securing equal angular rotation of the diameter about the axis, with every equal rise of temperature. These indications were at first extremely irregular. Ses I was able, however, to remove all ne; traces of irregularity by careful » Boao OiOle ss 3°83 mm. ie 248 PLANT RESPONSE The stimulus, it is to be remembered, is increased by increasing the voltage. But, on an undue increase of this charging voltage, to about forty volts or upwards, I have often found that the velocity undergoes an actual diminution. This is to be ascribed to the fact, which was demonstrated in the chapter on Polar Effects of Currents, that the excitatory value of the kathode reaches a limit, with a certain E.M.F., and that if the E.M.F. be carried far beyond this, the excita- tory effect is reversed, that is to say, it is now the anode that excites (p. 206). We have then the curious case of a negative direction, as it were, of transmission. For whereas, with moderate voltage, the excitatory disturbance travels in the interpolar region from kathode to anode, it now, with exces- sive voltage, travels in the opposite direction, from the anode towards the kathode. We can now see with what great accuracy it is possible to measure these changes of velocity, from which we can deduce the variations of conductivity, not merely qualitatively, but also quantitatively. This opens out to us the further possi- bility of studying the quantitative effects of various external agencies in modifying conductivity. I shall here relate a simple experiment which affords an example of the method to be followed in such an investigation. Effect of lowering of temperature on velocity of trans- mission.—In order to study the effect of lowered tempera- ture on conductivity, I applied ice-cold water over an area of 10 mm. of the conducting petiole in Bzophytum. The length of the conducting tissue experimented upon was 38 mm., and the time taken for the stimulus of a condenser-discharge (ten volts and ‘or microfarad) under normal conditions, z.e. before the application of ice-cold water, to traverse this length, was 101 seconds, giving a velocity of 3°76 mm. per second. But after the application of ice-cold water, the con- ductivity was so diminished that the transmitted excitation did not produce any response of the motile leaflet; on allowing the temperature of the cold water applied, however, to rise a few degrees, the stimulus was found to be effective ; TRANSMISSION OF EXCITATORY WAVES IN PLANTS 249 but the velocity of transmission was now found to be much reduced. Instead of 101 seconds being necessary for trans- mission through the entire length, it was now found to take 14°8 seconds. The difference of 4:7 seconds here, represents the additional time taken for transmission through the Io mm. length of cooled tissue. In other words, whereas transmis- sion through 10 mm. of normal tissue had taken about 2°6 seconds, it now took about 2°6 + 4'7, or 7°3 seconds ; that is to say, the conductivity was reduced by cooling to nearly one-third. Effect of rise of temperature on velocity.—It has been proved that conductivity is reduced by lowering of tempera- ture. We should therefore expect that a rise of temperature would produce the opposite, namely, an increase of conduc- tivity. That this is the case was shown by an experiment on a leaf of Bzophyium. Care was taken that the leaf should not be too young, since, as will be shown later, the effect of a rise of temperature on a young leaf is to initiate automatic response, I found that in this specimen excitation travelled a distance of 41 mm. in a centrifugal direction in 11 seconds, the temperature being 30° C. The velocity at this tempera- ture was therefore 3°7 mm. per second. On now raising the temperature to 35° C. the time taken for transmission was reduced to half, z.e. 5-5 seconds. The temperature was next raised to 37° C., and the time was now found to be further reduced to 4'5 seconds, the velocity being thus 9°I mm. per second, or nearly three times as great as at 30° C. TABLE SHOWING EFFECT OF RISE OF TEMPERATURE ON VELOCITY OF TRANSMISSION IN BIOPHYTUM Temperature Distance | Time Velocity | a | 30° 41 mm. | II seconds 3°7 mm. per sec. Bn 41 mm. 5°5 seconds 7°4 mm. per sec. Zu /e 41 mm. 4°5 seconds | 9*I mm. per sec. 250 PLANT RESPONSE Channels for conduction of effect of stimulus.-—Before concluding this chapter, it is important to consider the channels through which stimulus is conducted with the greatest facility. Since the conduction of stimulus is due to the transmission of protoplasmic change, it is clear, as already said in a previous chapter (p. 60), that such changes will be con- ducted most easily along those paths in which there is least interruption of protoplasmic continuity. It is evident, there- fore, that certain elements in the fibro-vascular bundles will furnish the best conducting medium for the transmission of stimulus. It also follows that in the fibro-vascular tissue itself, the conduction along the length would be more rapid and complete than across. On the other hand, the cells of indifferent tissue, such as the parenchyma of the leaf, are divided from each other by more or less complete septa, the fine filaments by which neighbouring cells may be protoplasmically connected being so minute that the conduction of stimulus through such im- perfect channels must be exceedingly feeble. These theoretical conclusions I have been able to verify by direct measurement of conductivity in different kinds of tissue. In this investigation, as motile tests of the state of excitability were not available, I devised an electrical method —to be referred to briefly in the next chapter, and described more fully elsewhere— by which to attack the problem. Using this method of investigation, | found that plant- organs which contained fibro-vascular elements, such-as the stem, peduncle, and petiole, were the best conductors of the state of excitation, and that conduction in such organs is much greater along the length than across it; in the peduncle of Musa, for example, the conductivity lengthwise is three times as great as that crosswise ; and finally I found that though indifferent tissues like the parenchyma are directly excitable, yet there is practically no transmission of that state of excitation through such tissue. From anatomical and other considerations, Dutrochet and Haberlandt came to the conclusion, that it was certain ele- TRANSMISSION OF EXCITATORY WAVES IN PLANTS 251 ments in the fibro-vascular bundle which were concerned in transmitting the disturbance in A/zmosa. This transmission was, however, regarded rather as a hydro-mechanical than as a true excitatory propagation. Such a conclusion, as we have already seen, appeared at one time to be probable in the light of the experiment on the trans- mission of excitation through a narcotised area. I have, however, already shown on p. 229, that abolition of motile excitability need not always imply the abolition of conduc- tivity. Haberlandt describes an experiment according to which the excitation in W/zmosa is said to have been propa- gated over dead tracts of the petiole, these portions having been destroyed by scalding. But it is extremely difficult to ensure the death of interior tissue by such means as super- ficial scalding. I have found that a portion of a plant-tissue when subjected locally to the action of boiling water, after- wards exhibited signs of true excitatory electric response. It is only by prolonged immersion in boiling water that one can be quite sure that the interior tissue is really killed by scalding, and unless this is done thoroughly it is easy to see that the inner cells may conduct the stimulus. There is, moreover, another possibility, that of pseudo- conduction, by which the effect of stimulus might appear to be transmitted across dead areas. My meaning will perhaps be clearer if we imagine two isolated muscle-preparations, one of which is attached to a striking lever, under which is the second. Supposing stimulus to be applied to the first of these, we can see that it would cause the lever to strike the second muscle, thus causing excitation. In this way, the effect of a stimulus applied to the first muscle would appear to have been transmitted to the second, completely isolated from it. In reality, however, this was not a case of true, but of pseudo-conduction, the excitation of the second muscle being started de novo by the blow of the lever, itself only a secondary effect of the excitation of the first. Similarly, we may have, in Haberlandt’s experiment, two living tissues isolated from each other by an intervening area 252 PLANT RESPONSE of dead tissue. A strong stimulus applied to the first of these will now cause an excitatory expulsion of water, which will be transmitted across the dead area, and impart a mechanical blow to the second living tissue, thus setting up excitation de novo in that portion of the petiole. Velocities of transmission in various plant and animal tissues.—I give below a number of determinations of velo- cities of transmission made with different plants, both ordinary and sensitive, the electrical method of determination having been used in the case of the former ; and with this, for purposes of comparison, a series of values that have been determined in the case of animal tissues. “The respective values given in the table refer to the maximum velocities obtained. In this connection, it should be remembered that the velocity of transmission depends on the intensity of stimulus. The intensity of stimulus, again, is diminished in the course of transmission through a long tract. Hence the velocity near the point of application of stimulus is relatively great, and becomes less the further the stimulus travels. In order, therefore, to make the different results comparable, my experiments have been made on equal lengths of tissue, namely, 7:5 cm. in each case, the stimulus applied being also the same. TABLES GIVING VELOCITIES OF TRANSMISSION OF EXCITATORY WAVE (a) Animal Subject | Velocity | Nerve of Anodon : - : : 10 mm. per second Nerve of Z/edone (observed by Uexkiill) . *5 to 1 mm. per second (6) Sensitive Plants L Subject Velocity Mimosa pudica: Petiole . ; , See 14 mm. per second Neptunia oleracea: Vetiole : ‘ ; I'l mm. ty Biophytum sensitivum : Petiole of direction centripetal : - 2°I mm. 3 Petiole of direction centrifugal. ; 3°38 mm. 49 Peduncle of . : . : é ; 3°7 mm. TRANSMISSION OF EXCITATORY WAVES IN PLANTS 253 < (c) Ordinary Plants Subject Velocity Ficus religiosa: Stem . ; ; . | 9'4 mm. per second Cucurbita: Tendril . F : . NO seaveay 5 ute; stems. : : : : E 3°5 mm. o Artocarpus: Petiole . : : ; “54 mm. + It will thus be seen that the velocity of transmission in conducting plant-tissue is not very different from that in the conducting tissue of certain animals. SUMMARY Successive determinations of velocity of transmission are consistent when the stimuli are uniform, and when _ inter- vening periods of rest, sufficient for complete protoplasmic recovery, are allowed. Velocities of transmission are not the same in centripetal and centrifugal directions. In Azophytum, for example, the centrifugal velocity is greater than the centripetal. When a given point in a plant-tissue is gradually raised in excitability, the consequent excitatory discharge takes place preferentially in one direction. Conductivity and excitability are both diminished by the increasing fatigue consequent on shortened intervals of rest. When the resting-period is shortened below a certain critical interval, the motile organ proves ‘refractory’ to further stimulus. The velocity of transmission is not the same for all intensities, but increases with increasing stimulation. The velocity of transmission is diminished by lowering, and increased by raising, of temperature. The fibro-vascular elements are the best channels for con- duction of stimulus : in them, the transmission lengthwise is greater than crosswise. Indifferent parenchymatous tissue has little or no power of conducting stimulus. The velocity of transmission in plants is not of an altogether different order of magnitude from that in certain animal tissues. CHAPTER “X21 ON DETECTION OF EXCITATORY PULSE DURING TRANSIT BY ELECTROTACTILE AND ELECTROMOTIVE METHODS Pfeffer’s experiment on expulsion of water from excited cells—Author’s experiment on a delicate method of detecting excitatory expulsion of cell-sap —Chemical method of determining velocity of transmission of excitation—Electrotactile detector—Demonstration of passage of excitatory contractile wave by means of electrotactile method—Determination of velocity of transmission of exci- tation in ordinary plants by electromotive method—Excitatory versus hydro- mechanical movement of water. THE fact that the excitatory wave is propagated with a constant and measurable velocity was demonstrated, with regard to sensitive plants, in the last chapter, the arrival of the wave from a distance at the motile organ being detected by means of mechanical response. As ordinary plants, on the other hand, do not possess such efficient motile indicators, some other method, more universally applicable, is necessary in order to show that in these also the state of excitation is transmitted from point to point. Before describing the two methods wiih I have devised for this purpose, I shall give an account of a very interesting experiment, depending on chemical reaction, by which I have been able not only to demonstrate in a striking manner the expulsion of water from excited cells, but also to make a rough determination of the velocity of transmission. It was shown by Pfeffer that on exciting the lower side of a pulvinus, water oozes out from the cut end. On taking a detached pulvinus, and stimulating the lower surface with a blunt needle, he found that the organ curved downwards, and a drop of water was seen to escape from the cut end, ELECTROTACTILE DETECTION OF EXCITATORY PULSE 255 Chemical method of detecting excitatory expulsion of cell-sap.—My own experiment differs from this in several particulars. Since there is a general impression that certain specialised tissues in the pulvinus are alone excitable, it was my object to show that cells which do not exhibit any motility will also give rise to the expulsion of water by excitatory contraction, and I desired further to utilise this effect for the determination of the velocity of transmission of excitation in that tissue. The essentials to this purpose were : some means of detection of the excitatory expulsion at the exact moment of its occurrence; some means of marking accurately the moment of application of stimulus; and, lastly, the use of a fairly long tract of tissue, in order that the interval between application of stimulus at one end, and the manifestation of its reaction at the other, might be of a duration capable of exact measurement. For this purpose I took petioles of Mimosa and the non-motile stems of the same plant, and placed their cut ends in very dilute solution of sodium chloride. So dilute a solution of salt does not, as I find, appreciably affect the excitability of the tissue. Selecting one of the specimens, say a petiole, I adjusted the electrothermic stimulator at a distance of, say, 4 cm. from its lower or cut end, the specimen being held vertical by means of a clamp. The vessel of salt solution in which it had hitherto been placed was now removed. The end of the petiole was carefully rinsed, to remove all traces of salt from the outside, and a small beaker of very dilute silver nitrate solution was substituted. At this point it became necessary to finish the experiment rapidly, as silver nitrate solution is likely after a time to affect the excitability of the tissue. Momentary thermal stimulus was given by brief closure of the electric circuit. The excitation then travelled through the intervening 4 cm. of tissue, with a velocity characteristic of the plant. When it reached the cut end, the excitatory contraction produced an expulsion of cell-sap containing the salt solution previously absorbed. This expulsion was instantly made visible by the formation of a dense white precipitate 256 PLANT RESPONSE of silver chloride. This was sometimes seen to be projected as a white vortex ring. The interval between the application of stimulus and this projection was found to be three seconds. It is thus clear that the excitatory wave is not one of hydrostatic disturbance, for such a disturbance would be transmitted with very much greater velocity. The velocity of transmission in this petiole is seen to have been 13 mm. per second, or practically the same as that obtained by a separate experiment on the fall of the leaf with similar specimens. In a similar manner I determined the velocity of trans- mission through the stem. The stimulus was applied at a distance of 5 cm. from the cut end, and the chemical pre- cipitation was observed, after an interval of five seconds. The velocity in the stem is thus seen to be 10 mm. per second. A repetition of this experiment with another piece of stem from the same plant gave similar results. It is thus seen that cells through which excitation is proceeding undergo excitatory contraction, in consequence of which there is an expulsion of water forwards, in the direction of propagation. And this effect is produced, not in pulvinated organs alone, but in others which are not usually regarded as motile. This result is also arrived at independently by the method of electrical response. The electrotactile method. —I shall next give an account of the much more delicate methods which I have succeeded in devising for the detection of the excitatory impulse during transit, the first of which is the electrotactile method. It is easy to understand that while the wave of excitation is passing through any given section of tissue, it must produce there certain form-changes, infinitesimal though they may be. Had our sense of touch been more delicate, we might perhaps have been able to perceive this pulse. It occurred to me, however, that it might be possible, if it existed, to obtain its indication by means of an electrical method of detection, the sensitiveness of which could be exalted at will. As regards the pulse of form-changes we can see that two ELECTROTACTILE DETECTION OF EXCITATORY PULSE 257 different effects are possible. For example, in the case of muscle with parallel fibres, the wave of excitation will travel onwards from the excited point, the contraction produced giving rise to expansion of the muscle in a direction at right angles to that of propagation. In the intestinal muscle, however, owing to a different distribution of the fibres, the propagated wave is one of constriction. Now, it is clear that these two kinds of muscle, placed within enclosing contacts, will give rise, during the passage of excitatory waves, in the one case to an increase of pressure, and in the other to its diminution. We are thus prepared to see that if similar contractile waves pass through a vegetable tissue, they may be detected by means of a concomitant variation of pressure—a variation which may prove to be either an increase, or a diminution, according to the particular disposition of the contractile elements in the tissue. Ifthe tissue, again, should happen to be anisotropic, the same wave of contraction may appear to give rise to a diminution of pressure in one direction, and an increase in that at right angles. In any case the excitatory wave, in the course of its transmission through any given area, might be expected to produce varzatzon of pressure, as between two diametrically opposite leading-points. I am now about to describe the electrical device which I have used for the detection of such transient pressure- variations, concomitant to the passage of excitation through vegetable tissues. It is known that the electrical resistance of contact varies with pressure, and on this principle depends the construction of the microphone. But a loose contact, such as would be favourable for microphonic use, is unsuitable for our present purpose, by reason of the disturbance to which it is subject from atmospheric vibration. The necessity to be met, therefore, is that of the adjustment of an electric contact, which shall not be subject to resistance-variation from atmospheric disturbance, and which shall, at the same time, be sensitive to the pressure-variation effected by the excited tissue. 258 PLANT RESPONSE I overcame the difficulty regarding extraneous dis- turbance by using, instead of a loose, a steady pressure- contact, capable of the finest adjustment, by means of a micrometer screw. The next problem lay in choosing contact material of great sensitiveness. For this purpose I used different materials, the sensitiveness of some being very great, while that of others was moderate. Carbon contacts belonged to the latter class, but had the advantage of being easily adjustable. Various metallic powders, how- ever, such as that of bronze, were considerably more sen- sitive, but at the same time required greater care for adjustment. . The most important factor in the arrangements, by which the sensitiveness of the contact may be exalted to a very high degree, lies in the proper adjustment of the electro- motive force acting on the contact circuit. The sensitiveness to pressure-variation increases with increasing electromotive force, being greatest when this is just short of a certain critical value, after which the electric contact is observed to become unstable, and give rise to spontaneous oscillatory variations. For the purpose of easy adjustment of the electromotive force I use a sliding potentiometer. The tissue, say a stem of J/zmosa, is placed between points, A and Bb, and by means of the micrometer screw, §S, it is pressed against the spring B. The electric contact, say of carbon points, is between B and C, which are in circuit with a galvanometer, and the potentiometer. The pressure is so adjusted that a feeble current flows through the gal- vanometer. In order to increase the sensitiveness of the detector, the electromotive force may be gradually increased, by proper adjustment of the potentiometer, short of the critical point (fig. 106). The deflected spot of light from the galvanometer will remain steady, provided the adjust- ments have been properly made. The tissue is now stimulated at a distant point, care being taken that it is not in any way jarred mechanically. Stimulation without jar is effected, however, without difficulty ELECTROTACTILE DETECTION OF EXCITATORY PULSE 259 by the use of the electrothermic stimulator. In any case, it is easy to discriminate between the effect of any such ex- traneous disturbance and the true excitatory effect in transit for which we are looking; for the effect of the former, if it f JIS i =i) i) 7) HH) Fpl | =) Lf Ae | 0 7d Fic, 106. Electrotactile Method for Detection of Excitatory Wave during Transit Stem placed under light pressure between B and sliding rod c. 4, 8, elec- tric contact points, the pressure of which is delicately adjusted by micrometer screw, M. A moderate current flows round the circuit, including the contact points, A B, and the galvanometer, G, the E.M.F. being suitably adjusted by the potentiometer slide, P Stimu- lation of stem effected by momentary closure of key, K, in circuit with electrothermic stimulator. Excitatory wave reaching the zone B, C, causes pressure-variation, with concomitant galvanometric response. occurs, is immediate, whereas that due to excitation takes place after a definite interval from the application of stimulus, In the experiment of which the record is given in fig. 107, stimulus was applied at a distance of 4 cm. The excitatory wave reached the experimental zone after an interval of five Ss 2 260 PLANT RESPONSE seconds, and indicated its presence by a diminution. of pressure on the contact (fig. 107), resulting in sudden diminution of current. After the passage of the excitatory wave the tissue was restored to its original condition, as shown by its once more exerting its normal pressure. This precess of response and recovery by variation of pressure is exhibited by the galvanometer record. The recovery is usually found to be attained in the course of a half to one minute, according to the intensity of stimulus. As the result of this experiment, we find the velocity in this particular stem to be 8 mm. per second, which is very near the determination already made by other methods, with different specimens of stem of M/zmosa. Frequently as I have obtained this response during transit by diminution of pressure, its opposite, that is to say, response by increase of pressure, is by no means uncommon. I have already explained how it is possible for excita- tory contraction to give rise to two Fue. 4O7 ao eicboneelé such opposed effects, in consequence Response in Stem of either of different dispositions of the eiicde . contractile elements in the tissue, or papas ae Fae es by the presence of anisotropy in the of 4 cm. below the organ. By means of the electrotactile detector. method, then, we are able to demon- strate the passage of the excitatory wave, and also to measure its velocity, in tissues which are not motile. The electromotive method.—I shall now describe the second, or electromotive, method which I have used for the detection of the excitatory wave during its passage through a vegetable tissue. I have already explained that when the plant-tissue is directly excited, the state of excitation is invariably accompanied by an electromotive variation, the excited point becoming galvanometrically negative (p. 32). Hence, when an excitatory wave is transmitted. through the ELECTROMOTIVE DETECTION OF EXCITATORY PULSE 26I tissue in any direction, from the stimulated point, there must always be an electromotive wave as its strict concomitant. The moment, therefore, when the excitation reaches a given point may be determined by observing the arrival of this excitatory electrical disturbance of galvanometric negativity, and for the detection of such an excitatory wave the gal- vanometer takes the place of the motile leaflet. In order to prove that the excitatory mechanical and electrical effects are strictly concomitant, it is only necessary Fic. 108. Experimental Arrangements for Simultaneous Recording of Mechanical and Electrical Responses Stimulus applied by thermo-electric stimulator, s, at A. Excitation reach- ing B causes mechanical response of leaflet, which is recorded by optical lever on drum at M. Simultaneous galvanometric negativity recorded at E. to perform an experiment on a plant, such as Lzophytum, which is provided with motile leaflets. We attach one of the indicating leaflets to the Optic Lever, and connect its base B with one of the electrodes of the galvanometer, the second electrode of which is connected with a distant point of the leaf (fig. 108). The two spots of light, one from the Optic Lever indicating mechanical, and the other from the gal- vanometer indicating electrical, response are adjusted to lie 262 PLANT RESPONSE one above the other on the same revolving drum. On applying a stimulus, say thermal, at A, it will be found, after the lapse of a definite interval, that both spots of light are deflected simultaneously, proving the concomitance of the mechanical and electrical effects. Such a record has been given already, in fig. 26. If we make a mark on the revolving drum at the moment of the application of stimulus, and a second mark when the electrical (and, in this particular case, also the mechanical) response is initiated, we can, with the previous knowledge of the speed of the drum, determine the time taken by the excitation to travel from A to B, and thus find the velocity of transmission for that specimen by electrical means. A further refinement of this method lies in the use of two galvanometers instead of one, the slight lag of response, caused by galvanometric inertia, being in this way eliminated. Particulars regarding this will be found elsewhere. It will be seen, then, that in the electromotive method we have a second means by which to determine the velocity of transmission of excitation in what are known as ordinary plants. I shall now describe experiments performed by this method. The peduncle of Azophytum is leafless. That it does transmit stimulus is seen nevertheless when we excite it at any point. Excitation will then be found transmitted through it to the main stem, from which it travels outwards to different leaves, a fact evidenced by the serial fall of leaflets in a centrifugal order. In such a peduncle I have determined the velocity of transmission by the electromotive method. The distance between the points A and B was 46 cm.; the time taken for transmission was 12°7 seconds. The velocity is thus found to be 3:7 mm. per second. Similarly, in the stem of /’zcus religiosa, | found velocity of transmission to be 9'4 mm. per second, which is almost the same as that in the stem of the so-called ‘ sensitive’ plant Wzmosa, From the experiments carried out on the electrotactile method it will be seen that excitation is conducted along a plant-tissue from cell to cell, as a contractile wave. We ELECTROMOTIVE DETECTION OF EXCITATORY PULSE 263 have also seen that water is expelled from an excited and therefore contracted cell. It is further clear that when an excitatory wave is proceeding in any direction, this cell-to- cell passage of excitation will give rise to a cell-to-cell contraction, the result of which will be a forward movement of water, which will have the velocity, not of hydrostatic transmission, but of the excitatory wave. The hydrostatic disturbance is quite distinct, being transmitted with great rapidity, and its presence has been shown in the preliminary abnormal response of erection in leaflets of Azophyturm (p. 24). . But that propulsion of water which is con- comitant to the passage of true excitation is very much slower, having the same speed as that of excitation itself. SUMMARY The direct effect of stimulus is not transmitted by means of hydrostatic disturbance, but by a cell-to-cell propagation of excitation. This transmission of excitation from ee) to cell is attended by a cell-to-cel! contraction. The passage of such a contractile wave may be detected by the electrotactile method, which thus enables us to determine the velocity of transmission of excitation, even in tissues which are not motile. In consequence of the concomitance of the excitatory wave with cellular contraction, water is moved forward pro- gressively, with a velocity and in a direction the same as that of excitation. This movement of water is not brought about by any hydro-mechanical action, but is the direct effect of the con- tractile wave due to excitation. The hydrostatic disturbance, when present, is transmitted with very great velocity, and its effect is seen in the abnormal preliminary response of erection, exhibited, for example, by the leaflets of Bzophytum. The velocity of transmission of excitation in ordinary plants may also be found by determining the velocity of the concomitant electromotive wave. CHAPTER. X20 THE LATENT PERIOD AND REFRACTORY PERIOD The determination of the latent period in A/zmosa—Experimental arrangements for obtaining automatic record — Prolongation of latent period by cold—Spark- record for determination of latent period—Prolongation of latent period by fatigue — Sluggishness of the response of Phz/anthus urinaria, also long latent period and very protracted period of recovery — Latent period reduced under strong stimulation—Response in Béophytum on the ‘all or none’ principle— Definite value of effective stimulus—Phenomenon of refractory period in Biophytum—Parallelism of responses in Azophytem and in cardiac muscle— Additive effects—Inappropriateness of term ‘refractory period ’-—Energy in excess of effective stimulus held latent for subsequent manifestation. HAVING explained the means by which it is possible to apply a quantitative stimulus of uniform or increasing intensity, and also how the responsive effect and its time- relations are accurately recorded, we shall next turn to the study of the various characteristics of the response itself, as given for example by the plant A/zmosa or Brophytum. In order that we may inspect different parts of the response- curve in greater detail, it will be necessary to take the record on a fast-moving drum, so that the curve may be drawn out, and its several features more easily distinguished. As we wish, moreover, to study the excitatory effect on the motile organ, stimulus will be applied directly on the pulvinus. Automatic method of record.—-As it will be necessary in the course of the following investigation to measure the times of reaction accurately, to small fractions of a second, the record must be obtained automatically. It is to be remembered that, as said before, response in vegetable tissues is relatively more sluggish than in animal, and it is super- fluous to arrange for measurements of time up to more than LATENT PERIOD AND REFRACTORY PERIOD 265 hundredths of a second. The experimental method that I am about to describe would, however, enable us, if necessary, to make determination of time-intervals of one-tenth this magnitude, the question being only one of using a recording drum with the requisite increased speed. The drum used for these experiments was one constructed by Verdin, and provided with a very perfect governor. Some little time elapses after starting the drum before it acquires uniform speed, which it afterwards maintains, however—at least during the short time required for the experiment—-with great perfection. The record is not taken until this uniform condition is attained. The mirror of the Optic Lever throws a spot of light upon a sensitive photographic film, wrapped round the revolving drum. In order to produce records of the required rapidity, I employ sunlight, proceeding from a pinhole, which after reflection from the mirror of the Optic Lever falls on the drum, appropriately focussed by means of a condensing lens placed at the end of a focussing tube, as seen in the figure. It is understood that, the record to be obtained being photographic, this experiment is carried out in a dark room, the sunlight required for the record being directed upon the pinhole by a heliostat outside. The stimulus consists of a single strong break-shock, from a Ruhmkorff’s coil, one electrode of which, by means of non-polarisable connections, is attached to the pulvinus of a leaf, and the other to the main stem lower down, of a speci- men of JZzmosa. The shock is applied by the recording drum itself, at a particular moment in the course of its revolution ; and at the same instant the curve of response begins to record itself automatically. These two acts—of imparting stimulus, and of opening a shutter by which the recording ray of light is allowed to fall upon the moving film —are performed simultaneously ; and both alike are initiated by the stroke of a rod which is fixed to the axis of the drum underneath. This rod, which I shall designate as the striker, at a certain period in the revolution of the drum, impinges upon a balanced 266 PLANT RESPONSE electric key, K,, thus closing an electro-magnetic circuit, and so releasing the shutter Ss, which is immediately in front of the pinhole, by which sunlight is admitted. This drop of the shutter, simultaneously producing a break of the Ruhmkorff’s coil circuit, gives an excitatory electrical shock to the plant. Fic. 109. Apparatus for Automatic Record When the speed of the drum has become uniform, Kk, is closed, and the striker, in connection with the drum, cleses the electro- -magnetic shutter circuit. The dropping of shutter, s, interrupts by kK, the primary circuit of the induction coil, the secondary of which gives a shock to the plant. The fall of ae leaf pulls down the Optical Lever, 0, pro- ducing record on drum. » Short-circuiting key of secondary. The electro-magnetic circuit of the shutter is interrupted by a key, K,, and this is kept open till the speed of the drum has become uniform. On closing k,, the circuit is still incomplete ; but the striker, impinging against the LATENT PERIOD AND REFRACTORY PERIOD 267 balanced key, K,, completes the circuit, and actuates the shutter. A thread connects the shutter with one arm of a second balanced key, K,. This arm is so overweighted that when freed, it causes two prongs, at the opposite extremity of the lever, which complete the primary circuit of the Ruhmkorff’s coil, to be lifted out of their cups of mercury, and thus the circuit is interrupted. But the thread is of such a length that when the shutter is set, so as to close the pinhole, the prongs, dipping into the cups of mercury, complete the primary circuit. The overweighted arm of kK, falls, with the drop of the shutter lifting up the prongs, and thus suddenly interrupts the primary current, giving rise to a break-shock in the secondary, which passes through the plant. During the course of the preliminary adjustment, when primary circuit of the coil is made, a make-shock is produced, but this is prevented from affecting the plant by a key, K,, which short-circuits the secondary. When the adjustment has been made, this short-circuiting key is opened. Briefly to recapitulate the procedure: The drum, carrying the sensitive film, is released, and begins to revolve. The key K, of the shutter-circuit is kept open, until a uniform speed is attained. It is then closed. The striker connected with the drum now closes the balanced key, k, ; the shutter drops, and simultaneously interrupts at K, the primary current of the induction coil, thus causing an excitatory shock to be given to the plant. The determination of the latent period.—It will be seen from the upper of the two photographs, given in fig. 110, that for a period of ;2,4, of a second the record remains hori- zontal. This represents the latent period, after which the tissue begins to respond. For a further period of half a second the leaf is seen to fall with a considerable and approxi- mately uniform speed. The rate of movement of the tip of the leaf is now 71 mm. per second. After this the leaf con- tinues to fall, but with a diminishing speed, till the maxi- mum contraction fall is reached. From records obtained on 268 PLANT RESPONSE slower-moving drums, I find that this is attained in different specimens, in a period of 1°5 to 2°5 seconds after the shock. This maximum contraction persists for a further period of about thirty seconds. The leaf now begins to erect itself, and full recovery is attained in the course of a further period of about six minutes. These statements refer to reaction in vigorous Mzemosa, at a favourable season of the year, like summer. In an unfavourable season, like winter, however, the reaction becomes very sluggish, and recovery is not then complete in less than eighteen minutes, or three times the normal period. Effect of cold on latent period.—-I shall next refer to the slug- gishness induced by cold, prolonging the latent period. The ex- treme instance of this is Fic. 110. Photographic Record of Response Seen when iced water is of AZimosa, Exhibiting the Latent Period aye =< Stee Tacit applied to the pulvinus, The upper curve was taken under normal and too great cooling conditions, and the lower when the pul- being thereby effected vinus was slightly cooled. Time-marks = ' ; ; = tenths of a second. Original record the response isabolished. atedecedtS Be eee ey ‘heaved i, With moderate cooling [The duplication, which will be observed in = each record, is due to the fact that the the latent period is found heliostatic mirror was silvered behind, thus producing two reflections, one from to be prolonged to the surface of the glass and the other from several seconds. This that of the silver. ] : effect cannot conveni- ently be shown, however, within the limits of a fast record. In order, therefore, to show the comparative effect of cold on the latent period, in the case of the same specimen whose record is seen in the upper of the two photographs in fig. 110, I was careful to cool the pulvinus very slightly. In the lower of these photographs it will be seen that the latent period has become prolonged, from the normal ,2, to 3°; of a second. 1 0 0 LATENT PERIOD AND REFRACTORY PERIOD 269 The rate of responsive movement is also seen to have under- gone considerable diminution. In the first, or normal, case, during half a second after the commencement of response, the rate of movement was, as said before, 71 mm. per second. In the second case, however, after slight cooling, it is seen to have been reduced to 26 mm. per second, or almost one-third of the original rate. Record by means of electric sparks: prolongation of latent period by fatigue.—In order to overcome the difficulty of the insensitiveness caused by keeping the plant in a photo- graphic dark room, I have recently devised a method of Fic. 111. Electric Spark Record, Showing Increase of Latent Period by Fatigue, in Successive Responses of a Leaf of AZzmosa Latent period in normal topmost record seen to be =5 second ; this increased in next—taken I minute before full recovery—to 3% second; latent period further increased in last case—taken 3 minutes before full recovery—to #5 second. Note also progressive change in slope of curve. record by means of a series of punctures produced by electric sparks on a recording paper surface. The sparks occur at the short gap between the end of the long arm of the record- ing aluminium lever, and the drum, these being connected respectively with the two electrodes of a Ruhmkorff’s coil. The electrical disturbance does not affect the plant, as the leaf is attached to the lever by a long silk thread. One great advantage of this method lies in the fact that the time- intervals, which may be as short as desired, are indicated by the distance between successive punctures, which are deter- mined by the frequency of the vibrating interrupter of the coil. In the case of which the record is given (fig. 111) the 270 PLANT RESPONSE interval between successive sparks was ; of a second. By this means, it was found that increasing fatigue induced a corresponding increase in the latent period of a leaf of Mimosa, from the normal ;25, to ;38, of a second. Response of Biophytum.—lI shall now proceed to de- scribe effects essentially similar to the last, seen in Bzophytum. In order to be able to observe in detail the various responsive peculiarities of the curve, subsequent to stimulation, records were taken in this case ona much slower-moving drum. The record given in fig. 112 shows the mechanical response to stimulation produced by discharge of condenser (capacity ‘ot 20” 30” 40” 50” Fic. 112. Response of Biophytum: Electrical Stimulus having been Applied at the Pulvinus of the Motile Leaflet The thick dot represents the moment of stimulation, microfarad, charged to nine volts). The exact moment of stimulation is marked on the record. It will be seen that the leaflet begins to respond almost instantaneously. The maximum contraction in this case, being considerably more rapid than in that of M/zmosa, is almost attained in the course of half a second. An interest- ing point to be noticed in the record is the flattening of the top of the curve (fig 112). That is to say, the maximum contraction persists for a considerable time before recovery begins. In the present case this lasted for ten seconds. This period varies in different specimens, from a maximum of ten to about two seconds. But in this particular specimen the period LATENT PERIOD AND- REFRACTORY PERIOD 27a in question remained at least approximately constant, in suc- cessive experiments. After this there was commencement of recovery, which was completed—as seen by the return of the spot of light to its exact original position—in the course of five minutes. In order to form an idea of the consistency of results which may be expected from a good specimen, I re- peated this experiment six times in succession, commencing the record each time at the same point on the recording surface as before. The degree to which all these curves coincided with each other in detail is almost incredible. Their rising portions, their flat tops, and their gradual descent during recovery, were all so coincident that the six successive curves appeared as but one. In specimens of plants which were not in good condition, fatigue was shown by the gradual diminution in height of successive responses. For accurate standard experiments it is therefore necessary to have specimens which are vigorous. We have already seen the prolongation of the latent period which is induced by cold, in the case of Mzmosa. I have obtained similar results also in working with Zvo- phytum. Yor example, in a certain experiment, moderate cooling induced a prolongation of two seconds in the latent period. When the plant was allowed to return to the sur- rounding temperature of the room, however, the increase of latent period disappeared. Latent period diminished by increased intensity of stimulus.—It has been said before that there are innumer- able gradations between the extreme cases of motile sensi- bility in plants. As regards motility, an extremely sensitive leaflet was that of Mzmosa pudica. Somewhat less quickly reacting were those of Azophytum sensitivum, and we had in the leaflets of Phelanthus urinaria an instance of extreme sluggishness. The latent period of the leaflet of Phz/anthus, under moderate stimulus, is as long as three minutes, and the maxi- mum contraction is not attained under forty minutes; but with a stronger stimulus the latent period is reduced to less Dz. PLANT RESPONSE than one minute, and the maximum contraction takes place in a relatively short period of about fifteen minutes (fig. 30, 6 and ¢, p. 44). Response of Biophytum on the ‘all or none’ principle. It is well known that in the case of a contractile skeletal muscle, there is a minimal intensity of stimulus which is necessary in order to produce contraction. From this point onwards, as the stimulus is gradually increased, the response increases, till a maximum contraction is arrived at, beyond which still further increase of stimulus produces no increase in effect. In cardiac muscle, however, the range of stimulus between minimal and maximal is practically narrowed to a point, so that the minimally effective stimulation is also at once maximal. It is to be remembered, at the same time, that the differences between cardiac and skeletal response are a question of degree, rather than of kind. Curiously, the response of Azophytum is, in this respect, somewhat similar to cardiac response. In an experiment with a particular specimen of Azophytum, the intensity of stimulus was increased by successive increments of the E.M.F. used for charging the condenser. With an E.M.F. of seven volts there was no response. With a charge of nine volts there was always a response, and this was maximal. A charge of eight volts was almost on the threshold of response. That is to say, when I started experimenting, the leaflet was in a somewhat sluggish condition, and an eight-volt. charge was ineffective. But after obtaining response to a nine-volt charge, I could obtain response also at eight volts. This was due to the fact that molecular inertness had been removed by the preceding effective shock. Thus we have two determi- nate values of stimulation, giving respectively maximum response and absence of response, the charges, namely, of nine and seven volts. The effective stimulus is, of course, constant for a given individual, but differs with the excita- bility of different specimens. Here, then, we have an instance of the ‘all or none’ effect. The leaflet either responds to the utmost, or not at all. LATENT PERIOD AND REFRACTORY PERIOD 273 Refractory period.—We shall next consider the pecu- liarities of the refractory period which I have discovered in the case of plant-tissues ; and for the material of this investi- gation we shall use the plant Azophytum, taking, in fact, the very specimen whose successive responses have already dis- played such remarkable consistency (fig. 112). Such uni- formity in successive responses is only possible when we allow sufficient time of rest for complete protoplasmic re- covery, by which the excitability is fully restored. But it has been shown in Chapter XX. that if sufficient time of rest be not allowed, the protoplasmic recovery is incomplete, and the excitability is diminished. Hence the extent of response, which is an outward indication of excitability, is diminished, and this effect is known as fatzgue, We also arrived, in the same chapter, at the theoretical conclusion that there is a minimum resting interval, the dimi- nution of which results in such a loss of excitability as to abolish response, and this period we know as the Refractory Period, because the leaf then apparently takes no account of stimulus, or is ‘refractory’ to it (fig. 105). We shall now enter into greater detail regarding the peculiarities of this refractory period. After taking the six curves in response to separate single stimuli which were so extraordinarily similar, I proceeded to take a curve of response to two equal stimuli of the same intensity as before—namely, nine volts, charging ‘OI microfarad—the two stimuli following each other at an interval of one second. The application of the second stimu- lus appeared to produce no effect, the extent and general character of response being the same as in the case of single stimulus, with only the difference of a slight elongation of the flattened top of the curve. I next tried the effect of two stimuli at an interval of five seconds. The leaflet was still refractory to the second stimulation, but when I applied it at an interval of ten seconds, the second stimulus became effec- tive. It will thus be seen that Lzophytum has rather a long refractory period, during which, as far as can be seen, it takes no account of the impact of a new stimulus. This refractory At 274 PLANT RESPONSE period is a matter of several seconds, but varies somewhat with different specimens. I have, again, in some cases observed a very curious phenomenon of two _ refractory periods. We thus find several very interesting parallelisms between the response of Bzophytum in plants and that of cardiac muscle in animals. We find in both that the minimal response is also the maximal, increasing stimulation pro- ducing no increase of response. In the response-curve itself the flattened top is common to both; both have a pro- longed refractory period ; and we shall see later that in both there is a tendency to the production of mul- tiple rhythmic responses. In all these respects the responses of SAzophytum resemble the cardiac re- sponses, rather than those of skeletal muscle. But they have one peculiarity Fic. 113. Additive Effects seen in Re- in which they share the sponses of Lzophytum to Stimuli which or Fall outside the Refractory Period characteristics of the re- The record to the left shows the effect of | Sponses of skeletal muscle. sae ene ag mamas of ones In the responses of cardiac muscle, successive effects are not additive, perhaps because that muscle undergoes the maximum contraction possible. In Azophytum, however, while any effective stinulus—whether minimal or largely super-maximal—will produce response of the same extent, yet this response, though the greatest possible for a single stimulus, is not the utmost of which the leaflet is capable. Hence, if we superpose successive stimuli, taking care that they do not fall within refractory periods, we shall obtain an extremely interesting response, showing the separate additive effects. I give here two curves, exhibiting these LATENT PERIOD AND REFRACTORY PERIOD 275 effects of superposition of stimuli. In the right-hand record in fig. 113 the stimuli were applied at intervals of thirty seconds. The successive responses, except the last, show a regular decrease. In the left-hand record, successive stimuli were applied at intervals of a minute, and appear much more equal. But even in these additive effects we find one peculiarity which is also characteristic of cardiac response. In the latter case, though on repetition of stimulus there is no summation of height of response, yet the apex-time of the second response is shorter than that of the first. We see this in the case of Azophytum, in the right-hand curve. The first response has a slightly rounded top, but this is reduced to an acute angle in the second. The record, having been reduced to one-eighth for reproduction, does not show this so plainly as does the original. Though each single response of Azophytum is maximal, yet from fig. 113 we have seen that this maximal response does not represent the utmost movement of which the leaflet is capable. In the particular plant here used for experiment, the maximal response to individual stimulus was always about thirty-eight divisions, but four superpositions produced a total movement of ninety-four divisions. It must be remembered that such effects can only be possible when the second stimulus is timed to fal] at the expiration of the preceding refractory period. If the absolute value of each individual minimally effective stimulus be represented by Ss, and if the whole be added together, or, in other words, if a stimulus of 4S be given at once, we may regard such a stimulus as made up of one minimally effective, p/ws three others which fall within the refractory period, and are thereby rendered totally ineffective. In other words, we may regard a very strong stimulus as made up of so many minimally effective stimuli. It is as if the first effective fraction alone acted, the succeeding portions, which arrive within the refractory period, being inoperative. 276 PLANT RESPONSE In view of certain other phenomena not altogether dis- connected, it seems unfortunate that the term ‘refractory period’ should be used with its present significance. For this term might be held to imply that the tissue refuses to take any account of the superfluous energy that is impressed- upon it. It is more likely, however, that by some peculiar mechanism, the superfluous stimulus—-z.e. what is over and above the amount necessary for producing maximal re- sponse—is prevented from overflowing. This excess of energy may, then, at least in some cases, remain latent, to be manifested at a later period in the form of excitatory impulses. Such impulses, again, attuned by some regulating process, may give rise to periodic or rhythmic overflow. That this is actually the case will be demonstrated in the next chapter. SUMMARY The latent period of response is protracted by cold. It is also protracted by fatigue. It is shorter under strong, than under moderate stimulation. In vigorous specimens of Azophytum leaflet, the mini- mally effective stimulus is also maximal; under normal conditions, this minimally effective stimulus has a definite value. There is also a definite refractory period in the response of Lrophytum. If a second stimulus fall within this refractory period, it appears to produce no effect. The response of the leaflet of Bzophytum resembles in many respects that of cardiac muscle. In both, response is on the ‘all or none’ principle, and both exhibit a relatively long refractory period. eA elo Ty, | t TIPLE AND AUTONOMOUS ~— ¢ RESPONSE GHAPTER XXIII ON MULTIPLE RESPONSE Multiple electromotive responses due to a single strong stimulus—Multiple electrotactile responses—Multiple mechanical responses in Azophylum— Cyclic variations in multiple responses—Multiple retinal excitations—Inter- mittent pulse in man and plant—Semi-automatism —Continuity of multiple and automatic response—Conversion of Szophytum into automatically responding plant; conversion of Desmodium into ordinarily responding plant—Similar polar effects of current in Avzophytum and in Desmodium leaflet, at standstill—Moderate stimulus in Avophytum and in Desmodium at standstill produces single response ; and strong stimulus, multiple response. I HAVE already explained in Chapter III. that the excitatory wave initiated by a stimulus has a concomitant electro- motive wave. If the plant experimented on is provided with motile leaves or leaflets, the excitation is evidenced by the simultaneous mechanical response of the motile organ, and the electrical response of galvanometric negativity. Multiple electromotive responses due to single strong stimulus.—In my investigations on electrical response in plants, I was surprised to find that whereas a single mode- rate stimulus gave rise to a single electrical response, a very strong stimulus very often initiated a multiple series of responses. I have obtained such multiple responses to a single stimulus with all kinds of plants, ordinary and ‘sensi- tive, and under the action of various forms of stimulus— chemical, thermal, and mechanical. To obtain these multiple electrical responses in an unmis- takable manner it is necessary that the excitation should reach one electrical contact and not the other, for in the latter case there would be complications arising from diphasic variation. The necessary condition may be fulfilled by 2580 PLANT RESPONSE applying stimulus near the proximal contact, the other contact being at a relatively great distance. The periodicity of these multiple electric responses I find to vary in different Fic. 114. Multiple Electrical Responses due to Single Strong Stimulus (az) In Mimosa due to thermal, and (6) to chemical stimulation ; (c) in” peduncle of Bzophytum, due to thermal stimulus. [N.B.—This series persisted for two hours.] (d@), in hypocotyl of 7amarindus indica, due to stimulus of cut. cases from about a half to five minutes. These multiple electrical responses, resulting from a single strong stimulus, sometimes persist for as long as two hours (fig. 114). Multiple electrotactile re- sponses.—In the course of my experiments on the electrotactile method of detecting the excitatory wave, I discovered multiple waves, initiated bya single strong stimulus, passing through the tissue. In fig. 115 I give a record of such multiple responses in the stem of Mimosa, in which it will be seen that there are four such responses, A A alas HIE CIO eit an wer tt average period of one aclile Response in Stem of Mimosa, due to Single Strong minute each. Thermal Stimulus Now, as these pulsations are signs of the presence of excitation, it follows from the occurrence of such multiple responses (Original record reduced to }.) that a strong stimulus may give rise to a multiple series MULTIPLE RESPONSE 281 of excitations. I was therefore led to expect that this fact might be demonstrated in another and still more con- vincing manner, if I should succeed in finding a sensitive plant which exhibited these multiple excitatory waves by repeated movements of the indicating motile leaflets. Certain peculiarities of Bzophytum which I had previously discovered led me to think that I might find in this plant the — opportunity I sought; for we have seen in the last chapter that in Bzophytum a certain minimal intensity of stimulus induced the maximal mechanical response. With such a stimulus we obtain only one response, and if we apply a stimulus of very much more than minimal intensity it produces no greater mechanical effect. What, then, happens to the excess of stimulus? This excess may be wasted as heat, or it may continue to exist in some latent form, and this latent stimulus may subsequently be given out rhythmically. Multiple mechanical responses.—In view of these facts I expected a strong stimulus to give rise to those periodic waves whose existence I had been led to suspect from the observation of the recurrent electromotive and electrotactile waves in various plants. As a matter of fact it has been noticed that Azophytum, when strongly excited, exhibits two successive movements of mechanical response, the leaflets not completing their closure at once, but in the course of two twitches succeeding each other. But I expected to detect a larger number of pulsatory movements in “response to a single strong stimulus. In order to do this, however, it was necessary to prevent the complete closure of the leaflets, by which the further exhibition of mechanical response was made impossible. For this purpose I used the Optic Lever, with the light counterpoise (p. 19). In this way I succeeded in demon- strating, through mechanical response, what had already been demonstrated electrically, the fact that a single strong stimulus, of whatever form—thermal, mechanical, electrical, or photic—will induce, not one but a multiple series of 282 PLANT RESPONSE rhythmic responses in a plant. Thus on applying a thermal stimulus to the petiole of Azophytum near its insertion on the stem, an excitatory wave was found, as usual, to produce successive depressions of leaflets in a centrifugal order. But after a while the existence of a second excitatory wave became evident, by a second series of closures of leaflets. That the wave was due—not to excitation reflected in some way from the tip of the leaf, but—to a second wave starting from the original point of stimulation, was made evident by the fact that the successive fall of leaflets was again centrifugal, from near the stem to the tip of the leaf. From the records now obtained by means of the Optic Lever, attached to one of the leaflets, it was interesting to observe the very numerous rhythmic pulsations, often as many as twenty, now made visible. As an instance of the simplest form of such multiple response, I give the following record, which was obtained under the thermal stimulation of the electrothermic stimu- lator. The point of application was close to the responding leaf. The average period of these pulsations was about thirty seconds (fig. 116), It may be stated here that the period of multiple responses of Azophytum is found to vary from about fifteen seconds to three minutes or so, depending on the condition of the plant and the intensity of the stimulus. The question suggests itself, since multiple electromotive and electrotactile excitations are observed in Mzmosa, why should not this plant also exhibit them mechanically? The answer to this is probably found in the fact that the Bzophy- tum \eaflet is light, and easily exhibits fluctuating impulses, whereas the impulsive fall of the heavier J7z7osa leaf persists, owing to greater momentum, till it is more or less complete. Again, unless an organ has at least partially recovered from fatigue, it is not susceptible of fresh excitation. The period of full recovery in the pulvinus of J/zmosa is very long, being about seven minutes. We saw, in studying fatigue in e) MULTIPLE RESPONSE 2 r =~ ~) Mimosa, that when a second stimulus succeeds a first, after an interval of less than one minute, it produces no responsive effect. If, then, a second wave of excitation arrives within the refractory period of the first, we must expect that it will remain mechanically ineffective. In the multiple electro- tactile responses seen in fig. 114, these successive excitations are seen to have occurred at intervals of one minute, com- plete recovery being accomplished in that time. In the Fic. 116. Multiple Mechanical Response of Biophytum, due to a Single Thermal Stimulus stem, then, the period of recovery is very much shorter than in the pulvinus, and the same waves of excitation which at intervals of one minute produce response in the one case, prove ineffective in the other. Had these periodic waves of multiple excitation been three or four times as slow as they are, we might have been able to observe multiple mechanical responses of the leaf of A/zmosa. In connection with this, I may state that I once observed a second mechanical response to a single strong stimulus 284 PLANT RESPONSE in the leaf of J/zmosa. In this case the second response occurred four minutes after the first. We have seen that multiple electrical response is obtained when any form of strong stimulation is employed. I now tried to find out whether multiple mechanical response could be produced by chemical stimulation. One of the middle leaflets of a Azophytum leaf was attached to the recording Optic Lever, and I applied a drop of sulphuric acid 1 cm. away from the recording leaflet in the direction of the tip of the leaf. This was found to give rise to five vigorous recurrent pulsations. Thus, though a single moderate stimulus evokes but a single response, yet under strong stimulation we obtain not only the immediate consequent response, but also a surplus of energy which remains over and is held latent in the tissue, to be given out later, after a shorter or longer interval, in the form of recurrent responses. From these experiments it is clear that a rhythmic series of effects need not have a periodic antecedent cause. As regards these pulsatory movements, it was shown on page 47 that the fall of the motile leaflet was due to a pulse of diminution, and its erection to a pulse of restoration, or increase, of turgidity. In these multiple responses, then, we have the expression of rhythmic variations of turgidity initiated by stimulus. Among ordinary responses—that is to say, single response to single stimulus—we have observed three types, depending on the excitability of the tissue. When the excitability remains uniform, the responses are uniform. When the excitability undergoes a gradual diminution, there is a cor- responding depression of response—that is to say, fatigue supervenes. And when the excitability increases by degrees, there is a correspondent enhancement of response known as the ‘staircase’ effect. In addition to these, I have, .as explained before, also noticed some curious instances in which the excitability of the tissue appeared to undergo periodic fluctuations, in consequence of which successive responses to MULTIPLE RESPONSE 285 uniform stimuli exhibited periodic fluctuations (p. 106). We may thus have an alternate, periodic, or cyclic fluctuation of excitability exhibited in the responses. In the first of these, the responses are alternately large and small. In the last, we may have either an ascending or descending series, which is periodically repeated. Cyclic variation in multiple response.—In multiple responses also we find all these types. And if the seat of origin of the multiple excita- tion be at a distance from the responding leaflet, we have in this fact an added element of variation. For we have now to consider, not simply the periodic variations of ex- citability of the motile organ, but also those of the excita- bility of the intervening tissue, Fic. 117. Multiple Response in Biophytum causing periodic changes of ; aoa : The rhythm, at first slow, becomes conductivity. Hence, in re- aiscken cords obtained of multiple responses, where the seat of excitation 1s at a distance, we have a complex combination of variations of amplitude and of period in the pulsations. Confining our attention to the period alone, we find that. multiple responses may be classified under three headings: Fic. 118. Multiple Response in 4z0phytum Quick rhythm becoming slow. those in which the successive periods are fairly uniform (fig. 116); those in which the pulsations are at first slow and later become quicker (fig. 117); and those which begin with 286 PLANT RESPONSE rapid pulsations, and afterwards slow down (fig. 118). And finally we may have any combination of these (figs. 119 and 120). Fic. 119. Multiple Response in Azophytum, showing Cyclic Groupings of Amplitude and Period These cyclic changes of amplitude and period are seen in all types of rhythmic multiple responses, of which I shall show that the autonomous responses of Desmodium and the growth-responses of all plants are only special cases. These characteristics are seen not only in the rhythmic responses of vegetable, but also in those of animal, tissues. The cause of the latter is still regarded as very obscure. Hence the study of similar phenomena in plants, under simpler conditions, may be expected to throw much light on the subject. How striking, even in their more intricate details, are the similarities between multiple responses in plant and animal, will be shown in the next three chapters. For the presenta Fic. 120. Multiple Re- ; sponse in Biophytum, shall confine myself to the considera- showing Cyclic Group- ings tion of two of the most obscure instances in animal tissues, and shail show them to be paralleled in the case of the vegetable. Recurrent visual impulses.—It is known that when we look for some time at a strongly illuminated object and after- wards close the eyes, we see the same image repeating itself many times in succession; and of this phenomenon, so far MULTIPLE RESPONSE 287 as I am aware, no satisfactory explanation has hitherto been offered. The phenomena which have been described in the case of vegetable tissues show, however, that this is nothing but an instance of multiple excitations in the retina caused by strong stimulation. Inthe case of the plant these recurrent excitations express themselves mechanically as twitches ; in the case of the firefly as flashes of light ; and in the retina as recurrent visual images. In these recurrent visual impulses are found all the peculiarities which I have observed in recurring excitatory impulses in Azophytum. This is shown, not only in simple cases, but also in the most complicated. I shall presently give, in illustration of this, two parallel instances of multiple excitation in Azophytum and multiple excitation in retina (see table, p. 288), in which the successive intervals undergo similar cyclic changes. In order to measure accurately the intervals between successive visual images, I use a special stereoscope, based on my discovery of the phenomenon of binocular alternation of vision, and by this means I have found that the after-effects of light in the two eyes are not simultaneous but alternate— that is to say, when the after-image in one eye is most vivid, that in the other eye has just vanished, and vce versa. For accurate measurement of this periodicity, it is necessary to have the effect on one eye distinguished from that on the other. For this purpose I have been accustomed to use a stereoscope containing, instead of photographs, incised plates with two inclined cuts, the right eye seeing the slit inclined to the right, and the left eye that inclined to the left. When the observer looks through this stereoscope turned towards a bright sky, his two eyes are acted on by strong stimulus of light. On closing the eyes, periodic visual impulses are sent from the strongly stimulated retina just as the stimu- lated area of Azophytum was found to send out periodic impulses.’ ' Bose, ‘ Binocular Alternation of Vision,’ Response zn the Living and Non- Living, p. 175. 288 PLANT RESPONSE TABLE SHOWING CYCLIC VARIATION OF MULTIPLE RESPONSES IN BIOPHYTUM AND IN RETINA Cyclic variation in the periodicity of the Cyclic variation in the recurrent visual multiple response of Brophytunz impulses ”) Interval between Ist & 2nd responses 52” Interval between Ist & 2nd responses 7’’ 5 4s 2nd ,, 3rd ae Cy ee c Pie 0 Are 250 a 5°25" * “A 3rd ,, 4th een ea =f sph gkde., 4m te 6°5"' Pe e 4th ,, 5th 55) eae - se eathys eben a, g “f + 5th ,, 6th ay gor ne 55 Sth, Oth 43 7/Ok i 5% oth .,,-7th Sis x aoe (OLDEas fen a5 peels In both these cases, therefore, we begin with a compara- tively long interval, which grows shorter, and then, again, is progressively lengthened. Intermittent pulsation.—We have now dealt with one of the two instances of correspondence between obscure pulsa- tory phenomena in animal and vegetable. The second, which remains to be considered, is that known to physicians as ‘the Fic. 121. Intermittent Human Pulse (Broadbent) intermittent pulse’ (fig. 121). ‘The term “intermittent” is employed to designate the pulse when a beat is occasionally missing from time to time, while the pulse in the intervals is perfectly regular. It is a remarkable variety of pulse, and is perhaps the least capable of explanation of any. The inter- mission may happen at regular and definite periods, every four, six, or up to twenty beats ; or the number of interven- ing pulsations may vary. The intermittent pulse may be habitual and constant, and in this case is more likely to be at definite intervals, or it may be occasional only, under the influence of some disturbing cause. . . . Occasionally nervous- ness and fatigue will render the intermissions more frequent.’ ! ' Broadbent, Zhe Pulse, p. 125. MULTIPLE RESPONSE 289 In connection with this, I have observed, among the types of cyclic variation in multiple response in plants, an instance in which every third response was missing, the period of each second beat being thus approximately twice as long as that of the first. It was easy to see that this was an instance of alternating fatigue, causing the particular record- ing leaflet to just miss the response every third time (p. 122). That the excitatory wave arrived in regular sequence, was seen by the fact that the neighbouring leaflets pulsated at regular intervals, Semi-automatism.—The plant Azophytum growing in the open, under favourable conditions of heat and light, some- times becomes so excessively sensitive that motile impulses are generated, the stimulus causing which it is often difficult to localise. A particular leaflet may have been moved by a puff of wind; or the alighting of a small insect, or the accidental grazing of an adjacent blade of grass, may have been the original source of the impulse. But this is enough to set all the leaflets of the plant quivering in an extraordinarily lively manner. For from the excited leaflet, the impulse travels inwards, the leaflets falling in centripetal succession. The excitatory wave then reaches the stem and overflows to the other leaves. But this time the progressive closure of the leaflets proceeds in a centrifugal or outward direction. Before the first discharge, however, going through the numerous avenues, can exhaust itself, the second impulse of the multiple response may begin ; and in this way the leaf- lets exhibit most lively movements without any immediate U 290 PLANT RESPONSE antecedent cause. In some of these cases the origin of the impulse can be traced, but there are others in which no external source of stimulus can be assigned. And here we find ourselves passing imperceptibly into the obscure region of automatism. There is much resemblance between such semi-automatic phenomena in Azophytum and the well-known instance of spontaneous movements exhibited by Desmodium gyrans, the successive mechanical responses of which exhibit all the peculiarities of multiple response as seen in Bzophytum. In Desmodium as in Biophytum we have similar cyclic changes of period and of amplitude. Just as in the case of Bzophytum, when we cannot determine the source of stimulus, we are tempted to regard the phenomenon as automatic, so in the case of Desmodium gyrans it is our own inability to trace out the origin of stimulus that leads us to regard the periodic movements of the plant as automatic. Biophytum, then, under ordinary circumstances, exhibits a single response to a single stimulus ; but when the stimulus is strong, a single stimulation will produce multiple responses, and these will persist long after the cessation of the primary stimulus. Under exceptionally favourable circumstances periodic movements occur, apparently without any exciting cause. I have again found, as will be described more fully in Chapter XXIV., that Azophytum itself under favourable cir- cumstances of light and warmth exhibits persistent and long- continued autonomous pulsations, which are in no way distinguishable from those of Desmodium. Even the periods of vibration are, generally speaking, similar, inasmuch as in both these plants, under different circumstances, I have recorded pulsations, the periodicities of which vary from one minute or less to four or five minutes. Thus Bzophytum forms a connecting link between those plants which exhibit only ordinary response (single stimulus, single response) and those in which mechanical movements appear to be auto- matic. iophytum is also particularly interesting, be- cause in its case we find the same plant, under different MULTIPLE RESPONSE 2901 circumstances, behaving in either of these ways; that is to say, giving ordinary response, or exhibiting automatic movements. Continuity of multipleand automatic response.— If there be thus no real breach of continuity between multiply and automatically responding plants, it should be possible, under suitable circumstances, to obtain from such a characteristi- cally automatic plant as Desmodzum all those peculiarities of responsive indication which were found in Lzophytum. The latter, it will be remembered, under the condition of over- excitability, consequent on the absorption of abundance of energy of heat and light, became converted from an ordinarily responding into an automatically responding plant. Con- versely we might expect Desmodzum, under the opposite circumstances, to pass from the condition of giving automatic to that of giving ordinary response. This inference I have been able to verify, for I succeeded in obtaining the requisite conditions for converting the auto- matically responding Desmodium into an ordinarily responding plant, in three different ways: firstly, by artificially reducing the absorbed energy of the plant, under cautious cooling, care being taken that this did not produce permanent cold-rigor ; secondly, by keeping a plant for some days in a dark room, at uniform temperature ; and thirdly, by selecting a plant for experiment inthe most unfavourable season of the year—that is to say, in the winter, when it was already exhausted by flowering. It will be seen that the principle adopted in each of these cases consisted in depriving the plant of its excess of energy. The third method was the most satisfactory of all, since the leaflets were in a state of natural standstill. In every instance, the automatic movements of the leaflets came to a stop, and the motile organ was reduced, as will be shown, to the ordinarily responding condition. In choosing the leaflets for experiment, it must be remembered that their sensitiveness is liable to disappear with age. In Azophytum, for example, very old leaflets show no response. In experi- menting, therefore, on leaflets of Desmodzum which have been U2 292 PLANT RESPONSE brought to a state of standstill, care should be taken to select those which have not permanently lost their motility, but in which automatic movements have simply come to a stop for want of a sufficient reserve of latent energy. Polar excitation of Desmodium leaflet in a state of standstill We have seen how the leaflets of Azophytum showed kathodic excitation at make. I shall now show that a Desmodium \eaflet, which has been brought to a state of standstill, either naturally or artificially, will exhibit similar effects of polar excitation ; and first, I shall take a case in which arrest was produced by cautious application of cold. I placed one electrode on the petiolule of the leaflef, and the other on the main petiole of the leaf, and applied the stimu- lus of condenser discharge, the petiole being made kathode. In this way I obtained from the Desmodium leaflet a series of single responses to single moderate stimuli. I next selected specimens which, in the winter season, had come to a state of natural standstill, and tried on them the polar effects of a constant current. Electrical connections were now made at the bases of the petiolules of lateral leaflets in two neighbouring leaves. In completing the elec- trical circuit, one of these leaflets would be under anodic, and the other under kathodic, action. I found that a consider- able electromotive force was necessary to initiate the excita- tory reaction. Thus, using thirty volts, I found that the kathodic leaflet responded at make, and the anodic at break. This shows that, as regards polar effects, Desmodium in a state of standstill gives exactly the same responses as does Biophytum. In the case of Bzophytum, moreover, we found that with high E.M.F., we arrived at a phase of response, the A stage, in which both the anode and the kathode caused excitation at make; and in Desmodium in a state of standstill I obtained an exactly similar result, for, on now using a higher E.M.F. of forty-eight volts, with the same specimens as in the last experiment, | obtained excitation at make, at both anode and kathode. MULTIPLE RESPONSE 293 Multiple response caused by strong stimulation in Desmodium.—I next tried to find out whether Desmodium in a state of standstill would give multiple responses to a strong chemical stimulation, as I had found Azophytum to do. Remembering how successive twitches are produced in a frog’s muscle, in a nerve-muscle preparation, when the nerve is touched with salt, I applied a strong solution of the same reagent to the petiolule of the arrested Desmodium \eaflet. This gave rise to a series of four vigorous mechanical pulsations. In order again to show that multiple response could be initiated by strong thermal stimulus, in Desimodiuwm as in Biophytum, 1 selected a plant whose leaflets were in a state of natural standstill. A fairly strong stimulus was applied by means of the thermal stimulator, at a point on the petiole half a centimetre above the insertion of the motile leaflet, in precisely the same manner as was ordinarily done with Biophytum. The Desmodium \eaflet now gave a multiple series of responses, exactly similar to those obtained from Biophytum. The first occurred three and a half minutes after the application of stimulus. The successive rise and fall were then uninterrupted. The average period of each response in the series was approximately 4°5 minutes. The multiple responses gradually declined in amplitude, and came to a stop after the thirteenth oscillation. It is thus seen that a strong stimulus will give rise to a multiple series of re- sponses in the case of Desmodium, precisely as in that of Biophytum. It is now clear that there is no rigid line of demarcation between multiple and automatic responses. An ordinarily responding plant like Azophytum, which gives a single response to single moderate stimulus, and multiple response to strong stimulus, will, under very favourable circumstances, that is to say, when it has absorbed an excess of energy from without, become automatically responding ; and, conversely, the pro- nouncedly automatic Desmodium will, under unfavourable circumstances, that is to say, when the sum total of its latent 204 PLANT RESPONSE energy has fallen below par, be reduced to the condition of an ordinarily responding plant, giving single response to single moderate stimulus, and multiple response to strong stimulus. SUMMARY On application of a strong stimulus, of whatever nature, to a vegetable tissue, a multiple series of electromotive re- sponses is produced. These multiple responses may also be observed by the electrotactile method. These multiple excitations, in consequence of a single strong stimulus, may be observed in Azophytum as multiple mechanical responses. These multiple responses may be uniform in character, or may exhibit cyclic variations, similar to those observed in the rhythmic pulsations of animal tissues. As, in the case of a plant-tissue, a strong stimulus causes multiple excitations, so, in the retina, strong stimulus of light causes multiple visual excitations, seen in recurrent after- images, which have the same characteristics as the multiple after-effects of stimulation in Bzophytum. There is no strict line of demarcation between the phenomena of multiple and of automatic response. Under very favourable circumstances—that is to say, when it has absorbed an excess of energy from without—an ordinarily responding plant like Azophytum will become converted into an apparently automatically responding plant, like Dessmodzum. Conversely, under unfavourable circumstances—that is to say, when the sum total of its energy is below par—an automatically responding plant like Desmodium, will become converted into an ordinarily responding plant like Azophytum. Its leaflets then come to a state of standstill. Desmodium \eaflets in a state of standstill respond to stimulus in exactly the same way as do those of Bzophytum. ‘To moderate stimulus, both give single response ; the polar effects of currents in both are the same; and strong stimula- tion causes a multiple series of responses in both. CHAPTER XXIV AN INQUIRY INTO THE CAUSES OF AUTONOMOUS MOVEMENTS Production of pulsatory movements as after-effect of energy absorbed—Physical analogue—Localisation of seat of automatic excitation in Desmodium— Demonstration of multiple response to a constant stimulus : (1) Chemical-— (2) Electrical—(3) Stimulus of light—Multiple response to constant stimulus of light, in: (a) retina—(4) Brophytum—(c) Desmodium—(4) Thermal— Induction of automatism in Szophytum at favourable temperature—(5) Of internal hydrostatic pressure—Absorption of external energy and its absorption by the plant in latent form—True meaning of ‘ tonic’ condition—Cause of rhythmicity—After-effect, and its relative persistence. In living tissues, both animal and vegetable, we find numerous cases of spontaneous periodic movements, to which no direct exciting cause is apparently assignable. We confess our inability to trace out the exciting cause by classing such phenomena as automatic. Among well- known examples of automatic movements in animal tissues may be cited the pulsatory action of the heart. In the vegetable kingdom, also, such movements are very numerous, and are of various degrees of rapidity, from quick pulsations of some few seconds in duration, to others which occupy periods of several hours. It is to be remembered that these spontaneous movements take place in plants under favourable circumstances, z.é. under that totality of the optimum degrees of light, temperature, turgidity, and so on, which is vaguely referred to in vegetable physiology as the fomzc condition. 1 intend to set forth presently experimental considerations which will, I hope, serve to make clear the precise significance of this term. As already said, to call such movements automatic is 296 PLANT RESPONSE only one way of evading the difficulty of finding out their actual cause. When we see these responsive indications given by a moving leaf or leaflet, we can but feel it necessary to trace them to the impulses, internal or external, by which they must have been occasioned. How are such periodic impulses caused, and where is their seat? The investigations which follow are intended to throw light on this, one of the most obscure problems in Physiology. In previous chapters I have demonstrated the fact that the antecedent cause of a periodic effect need not itself be periodic. A stimulus may remain long latent in a tissue, and this latent stimulus may subsequently give rise to periodic excitations. Such periodic expression of the absorbed energy is not without analogy in the world of physics. For example, we take a glass tube of moderate diameter and push into it, toa certain distance from the free end, a piece of wire gauze, which is then heated over a Bunsen burner. On removal of the heating flame, we observe the after-effect of this absorbed thermal energy, in pulsating movements of the air-column, which give rise to a musical note, whose pitch is determined by their periodicity. We have here a physical analogue to a case previously described, in which the thermal energy absorbed by a tissue of Bzophytum was afterwards manifested in long-continued periodic movements of the leaflets. In the experiment with the glass tube, the periodicity of aérial pulsation is determined by the size, shape, and temperature of the pulsating column. Similarly, the periodicity of multiple response in the Bzophytum leaflet is determined by the various constants of the cell-complex which isthe seat of the movement. We saw in Azophytum that the region to which strong external stimulus was applied, became the place in which it was accumulated, and the source of subsequent excitation. We further saw that there is no rigid line of demarcation between plants which exhibit multiple responses and those which show autonomous movements, the same plant passing from one category to the other, according to circumstances, INQUIRY INTO CAUSES OF AUTONOMOUS MOVEMENTS 297 Thus Azophvtum, which normally speaking gives ordinary single or multiple response, may under favourable circum- stances exhibit automatic response. On the other hand, Desmodium, which normally speaking exhibits automatic response, will under unfavourable circumstances become converted into an ordinarily responding plant. As already stated, in the case of Bzophytum, the point of application of stimulus becomes also the source of subse- quent multiple excitations. An observer, unacquainted with the position of this point, might succeed in determining it, by watching the order of pulsation of the leaflets. For if we suppose the stimulus to have been applied at some point in the middle of the leaf, the observer will in that case notice periodic waves of excitation proceeding in opposite directions, and giving rise to the closure of successive leaflets from a common point outwards, thus clearly indicating the position of the point from which successive excitations are initiated. Had the stem, on the other hand, been stimulated, and thus become the source of excitation, these successive impulses would have begun by arriving at the first pair of leaflets, and thence would have passed through all the leaflets to the tip of the leaf, in a centrifugal order. Again, had the stimulus remained latent at the tip of a particular leaf, the successive excitatory waves would then have proceeded through the leaf in a centripetal order, and on reaching the stem would have radiated outwards, or in a centrifugal sequence once more, throughout the other leaves. There are other ways also, presently to be described, by whose means we can obtain an idea of the direction in which the excitatory wave is travelling. Localisation of seat of origin of autonomous excitation in Desmodium.—Our next inquiry is into the very obscure question of the point of origin of the so-called autonomous excitation of Desmodium. I have already shown that, if the plant absorbs a certain amount of energy in excess of that required for immediate response, the surplus is stored up, to be given out subsequently in the form of pulsating waves of 298 PLANT RESPONSE , excitatory disturbance. But the difficulty is to determine the point at which the latent energy is stored up, and from which the excitatory disturbances subsequently proceed. As we are accustomed to think that stimulus must be due to some sudden variation, one is tempted to suppose, as the simplest explanation, that in the case of autonomous move- ments the stimulation is caused by variations in the rate of absorption of food material, which is probably being carried on, in an intermittent manner only, by the roots and leaves. In this case, if the stimulus proceed from the root, we may expect the excitation to travel to the motile leaflets through the stem, outwards, or in a centrifugal direction. If the assimilating leaves, however, be the source of excitation, we shall look to see the wave of excitation proceeding from without inwards, or centripetally. In order, therefore, to localise the source of stimulation in Desmodium gyrans, for example, I first tried to find out in which direction the excitatory impulse proceeded. Unfortunately in this case, the leaf being provided with only a single pair of motile leaflets, it is impossible to obtain the indication of direction which is given by Azophytum, by the successive closure of leaflets in a definite order. I was therefore obliged to have recourse to other expedients. I have already shown how the transmission of stitnulus can be arrested by local application of ether, or by the anodic block. Ifthe pulsating stimulus, therefore, should be central, that is to say, proceeding from the stem, we should then expect the application of ether, or the anodic block, at the junction of the leaf with the stem, to arrest the movement of the leaflets. If, on the other hand, the source of the stimula- tion should be peripheral, a block produced in the manner described, at a point between the terminal expanded leaflet and the small motile leaflets, would prevent the response of the latter. In carrying out these experiments, however, I found that no such arrest took place in either case. The fact that the source of stimulation is not central, is also made evident by the following consideration. Had it INQUIRY INTO CAUSES OF AUTONOMOUS MOVEMENTS 299 been so, the responding movements would have been syn- chronous with that of the hypothetical source, and all the leaflets of the plant would in that case have had the same period of vibration. As a matter of fact, however, different leaflets have different periodicities. That the source of stimulation, again, is neither central nor peripheral, may be shown by applying two tight liga- tures, one behind and the other in front of the motile leaflets. The passage of the excitatory impulse, should this be transmitted from either direction, will by this means be completely arrested. On carrying out this experiment, how- ever, the periodic pulsation was not affected. Again, the leaf may be completely detached from the plant, and the terminal leaflet amputated. But the periodic pulsation still proceeds as before, just like the persistent beat of the isolated heart of a frog. Thus we find that the isolated motile leaflet continues to manifest an evolution of energy in the form of pulsating movements, which cannot be derived from either of the hypothetical sources, whether central or peripheral. It maintains this activity for a long time, when kept under favourable conditions. It thus becomes clear that in the case of Desmodium the power of maintaining rhythmic pulsation is local, and resides in the tissue of the motile leaflets. This does not exclude the possibility of other periodically moving organs obtaining the pulsating excitatory impulses from a distant point. Such a case may well happen when, the conductivity of the intervening tissue being very great, and the excitability of the motile organ high, stimulation, though enfeebled by transmission through a long tract, yet remains above that critical intensity which would cause effective response. In the previous chapter, I showed that if Desmodium be kept in the dark for a sufficiently long period—but not so long as to produce permanent rigor—or if a specimen be taken in the unfavourable season of the year, its autonomic movements would be found to have come to a stop for the 300 PLANT RESPONSE time. This was because the energy stored up in the tissue had become exhausted. If, then, a single stimulus were given, say, by condenser discharge, a single response would be found to ensue. This showed that in such a case the arrest of the automatic movements was not due to the abolition of motility in the leaflet, but only to exhaustion of the energy stored up, which would have given rise to oscilla- tion. It was also shown that if Desmodium in a state of standstill were subjected to a single strong thermal stimulus, it exhibited a multiple series of rhythmic responses. Multiple response to constant stimulus. — Having observed the production of multiple responses as an after- effect of a single strong stimulus, I shall now proceed to demonstrate the production of similar multiple responses by the action of a constant excitatory condition. 1. Chemical—tI have shown that in Azophytum, and in Desmodium at standstill, rhythmic pulsations are produced by the constant action of a chemical stimulant. Analogous phenomena are also known in animal tissues ; in the isolated heart in a state of standstill, rhythmic pulsation can be renewed by chemical stimulation. 2. Electrical—I\ have often observed that when a strong current is sent through Averrhoa carambola, a number of rhythmic pulsations are found to take place. In animal tissues, again, similar rhythmic excitations have been ob- served, not only in cardiac muscle, whose rhythmicity is so marked a characteristic, but also in skeletal and other muscles, 3. Stzmulus of light: (a) On retina—And now before I describe the experimental demonstration of periodic excita- tion in plants, as caused by constant stimulus of light, I shall refer in detail to certain remarkable periodic effects which I have observed in the retina, under the action of the same stimulus. I showed in the last chapter that strong stimulus of light gives rise in the retina to multiple responses, in the form of recurrent after-images. I shall now prove that during the continuance of constant light, pulsatory visual effects are INQUIRY INTO CAUSES OF AUTONOMOUS MOVEMENTS 301 produced. These pulsations are not usually noticed in visual sensation, owing firstly to the absence of a standard of com- parison, and secondly to the fact that though the impressions on the individual retina: undergo variation, the sum total of the two remains constant. I have been able to provide the neces- sary comparison-standards by having two distinguishable images produced in the two eyes, the fluctuation in the visual excitation in one eye being thus capable of detection by com- parison with that in the other. It would have been impossible to detect this fluctuation had the excitatory variation taken place in the two eyes simultaneously, ze. if the maximum excitation in the one had occurred at the same moment as the maximum excitation in the other. But I have found that, as regards excitation, there is a relative difference of phase, of half a period, between the two eyes, so that the maximum effect at the given moment in one eye corresponds to the minimum inthe other. It is owing to this fact that the periodic excita- tions in each retina are brought out in an unmistakable manner by the following experiment, which consists in look- ing at two slits through the modified stereoscope described in the previous chapter. One of the two slits inclines to the right and the other to the left, and on looking through these at a bright sky, the right eye perceives a bar of light turned, say to the right, and the left eye a bar turned to the left, the resultant impression being that of an inclined cross. When the stereoscope is turned to a bright sky, and the cross looked at steadily for some time, it will be found, owing to pulsatory excitation in each eye, that when one arm of the cross begins to be dim, the other becomes bright, and vice versa, ‘Vhese periodic fluctuations are perceived con- tinuously under the constant action of light.! I shall now proceed to the demonstration of the very interesting rhythmic movements caused in plants under constant light-stimulus. As we wish to prove that the cause of automatic movements lies in the action of some 1 The experiment will be found described in detail in my book, Response in the Living and Non-Living, p. 175. 302 PLANT RESPONSE continuous stimulation, it is necessary to satisfy ourselves that this continuous source is the sole cause of the rhythmic movements. This will appear conclusive, if it is made clear that the plant does not possess any intrinsic energy of self- movement, but only the power to regulate rhythmically the overflow of energy supplied. The demonstration will then be made rigorous, if we take a plant which is not manifesting automatic movement, and cause it to exhibit such action by the application of constant external stimulus. For the purpose of this experiment, then, we may take the leaflet of Desmodium in a state of standstill, or that of Bzophytum under normal conditions, which, as we have seen, may be regarded as practically equivalent to Desmodium in a standstill condition. If therefore we can succeed in making Azophytum exhibit rhythmic movements under the continuous action of a given external energy, we shall not need to look for the explanation of the so-called automatic movements of Desmodium to some periodically varying stimulus, any constant stimulus being then proved fully competent to produce the rhythmic pulsations. (0) On Biophytum.—I first subjected the motile leaflet of Biophytum to light of comparatively short duration, ze. two minutes, by throwing sunlight upon it from a reflecting mirror. There was no immediate response, but after a latent period of one minute the leaflet gave a single response. I next subjected the plant to the continuous action of light during ten minutes. I obtained rhythmic pulsations, there being two responses in the given time. I then cut off the light, and the rhythmic action was stopped. After an interval of several minutes I again applied light for ten minutes, and this gave rise to similar rhythmic responses. The procedure was repeated once more with the same results. With another specimen, I applied light for a still longer period, ze. twelve minutes, and I obtained three pulsations. It will thus be seen that we have here a regulated outflow of energy ; the plant absorbs energy continuously, but gives it out in a pulsating manner (fig. 123). In this way, a number INQUIRY INTO CAUSES OF AUTONOMOUS MOVEMENTS 303 of autonomic pulsations may be obtained in Azophytum under the continuous stimulation of light. The recovery not being complete after each pulse, the result is a progressive folding downwards of the leaflets, and this makes it impossible after a time to observe further responses. I shall presently describe, however, another method of supplying the plant with energy, by thermal means, in which case the autonomous pulsation in Azo- phytum is prolonged indefinitely. (c) On Desmodium leaflet at standstill Turning now to the leaflet of Desmodium in a state of standstill, I find that application of sunlight initiates rhythmic move- ments. Ina particular experiment, light was continuously applied for half an hour, and there were pro- duced in that time seven vibrations, The responses were found to under- go™ staircase’ increase by increas- Fic. 123. Multiple Response : 4 of Sophytum under the Ing absorption of energy, and the Continuous Action of Light resultant increase of molecular mobility produced in the tissue. On the stoppage of light the rhythmic pulsations persisted for some time, the ampli- tude, however, undergoing diminution owing to the run-down of absorbed energy. 4. Thermal: (a) Induction of autonomous response in Biophytum.—I shall now describe an experiment of very great theoretical importance, by which I have able to convert a specimen of Azophytum from an ordinarily into an auto- matically responding plant. Guided by the theoretical -in- ference which I have already stated, that it is an excess of energy that brings about the condition of automatism, I subjected a specimen of Azophytum to the constant stimulus of a moderately high temperature. I first placed the speci- men in a chamber whose temperature was 37° C., and it was ‘ astonishing to see the younger (and therefore more excitable) 304 PLANT RESPONSE leaflets, at first quiescent, break into a sustained series of uninterrupted pulsatory movements, which they kept up throughout the maintenance of the condition of high tempera- ture. It was interesting also to observe the quickening of vibration by absorption of energy. The pulses were at first slow, each having a period of four minutes, but they became steadily more rapid, till they had reached a frequency of two vibrations in a minute. A continuous record of these periods was made during one hour, and the following tabular state- ment exhibits the results : TABLE SHOWING PERIODS OF SUCCESSIVE PULSATIONS IN BIOPHYTUM WHEN TEMPERATURE IS RAISED TO 37° C. | | | te Period | Aas ’ Period ! pa Period a | I 4 minutes (esa T 2°5 minutes 21 *5 minute 2 4 >» 12 2°75 | 22 eye “re 3 at Ss reas 2°75 és | 23 Care 4 | 4 ” | 14 2°5 ” | 24 5 9? 5 4 ” 15 2 9 25 | 5) ” ay | 3 5 | 26 | 1°5 minute 26 cesar 74 | 3 ” 17 I°5 ” 27 5 ” 8 | 3 2? | 18 I 9 | 28 5 ” 9 3 ” 19 | I ” || 2 5 ”? 10 | 2°51» | 20 | 25) 9 ars} 5 ” | Having thus found that a high temperature was favour- able to the initiation of automatic movements in Bzophytum, I was next desirous of determining the minimum temperature at which such responses could be induced. For this purpose I took a fresh specimen of Azophytum, and cautiously raised the temperature of the chamber from 23° C. upwards. When 29° C. had been reached, I obtained the first pulse of auto- nomous response, the period being rather slow—that is to say, 85 minutes—and by the time the temperature had gone up to 35°C. this period had become shortened to two minutes. We find here a phenomenon identical with that of Desmodium, where the frequency of vibration is found to be increased with rising temperature. INQUIRY INTO CAUSES OF AUTONOMOUS MOVEMENTS 305 In Desmodium, the autonomous movement is initiated at a certain more or less definite temperature, which is about 17° C. This we may call the critical thermo-tonic condition. Below this critical degree, Desmodium ceases to be autonomic, and becomes an ordinarily responding plant. In Bzophytum, similarly, the critical thermo-tonic point is about 29° C. Above this, the young leaflets are autonomic, and _ below it, ordinarily responding. The difference between Desmodium and Bzophytum in this respect lies, therefore, in the fact that their critical thermo-tonic points are about twelve degrees apart. In the case of Bzophytum, when the temperature is main- tained at a uniform favourable degree, the periods of the Fic. 124. Induction of Autonomous Response in Bzophyteum, at Moderately High Temperature of 35° C. Note the diminution of amplitude of response with the gradual loss of latent energy, consequent on falling temperature. The pulsations came to a stop below 29° C. autonomous pulsations become very regular. It has been said that these pulsatory movements are maintained by means of energy absorbed, and with Bzophytum 1 found an added opportunity of demonstrating this fact. A particular plant had been kept at a uniform temperature of 35° C,, under which the young leaflets gave autonomous responses. The heating current, by which this temperature was main- tained uniform, was now stopped, and the chamber gradually x 306 PLANT RESPONSE cooled down. One of the young leaflets had been attached to the Optical Lever, and records were now taken con- tinuously. It was very interesting to observe how, with the gradual'loss of absorbed energy, the pulsating movements of the plant became diminished in amplitude (fig. 124) till they came toa stop. We shall find exactly the same thing in the case of Desmodium. A I referred in the last chapter to certain observations of my own, in which the leaflets of Bzophytum were found, under favourable circumstances of light and temperature, to give rise to apparently spontaneous movements, which could not be traced to any definite varying external cause. From the experiment just described, however, we see that it was the continuous stimulus of favourable temperature and light com- bined that caused this rhythmic movement, which appeared as automatic. (6) Resumption of automatic movements in Desmo- dium.—Similarly in Desmodzum, if the isolated leaflet in which all movement has been brought to a stop, but not to a condi- tion of permanent rigor, be raised in temperature, and if the tissue be maintained uniformly at this higher point, the pulsatory movement will be found to commence and persist for a long time. The initiation and maintenance of the responses here, again, is undoubtedly due to the renewed supply of energy. A new difficulty now arises, however, from our habit of regarding stimulation as dependent upon some sudden variation of external conditions. For we are here confronted with a case in which uniform and continuous application of heat produces stimulation. It may be urged that there has been some variation of environmental conditions, in the fact of the rise of temperature before the constant point was reached. But it has to be remembered, that this rise was purposely made very gradual, and even if a single stimulation had been caused by this preliminary thermal variation, it would have evoked only a single response, or at most a few multiple responses But how are we to account for these INQUIRY INTO CAUSES OF AUTONOMOUS MOVEMENTS 307 long-continued periodic pulsations, which are kept up during the whole time in which the leaflets are maintained at an unvarying given temperature? We are thus led to see that stimulation is not in all cases dependent upon the occurrence of a sudden change in the exciting cause. On the contrary, excitation may be pro- duced, as we have seen, under a constant and uniform exciting condition. If we analyse the multiple responses produced in Bzophytum as the after-effect of the application of a strong thermal stimulus, we find, as said before, that some part of this thermal energy remains latent, and after- wards gives rise to recurring pulsations. 5. Of internal hydrostatic pressure.—The isolated animal heart, when in a state of standstill, is found to renew its excitatory pulsation under an increase of internal hydrostatic pressure. I shall show later (p. 349) that a Desmodium leaflet, similarly, when in a state of standstill, can be made to resume its excitatory rhythmic activity, by increasing the internal hydrostatic pressure. The true meaning of ‘tonic’ condition.—We have thus seen that under the continuous action of a constant source of external stimulus, multiple responses are produced. We have also seen that the excess of energy absorbed remains latent in the tissue, in consequence of which, even on the cessation of external stimulation, the pulsatory movements are main- tained for a longer or shorter time. It is thus the excess of stimulus absorbed which renders the tissue excitable, or ‘tonic. Hence we may have tonicity imparted by light, photo-tonus; by favourable temperature, s¢hermo-tonus; by electric current, e/ectro-tonus ; by internal hydrostatic pres- sure, iydro-tonus ; or by the presence of favourable chemical] substances, chemo-tonus. We have seen that each one of ‘these, by itself, was competent to give rise to multiple responses. It has been shown further that there is no hard- and-fast line between such multiple and automatic responses, the one passing imperceptibly into the other. We have now seen that the tonic condition of the plant is x 2 308 PLANT RESPONSE determined by the sum total of the latent energy derived from all the above-named factors. This internal factor, of latent energy, will be shown to play a very important part in all response-phenomena, and as it thus becomes necessary to have some convenient means of referring to it, we shall henceforth designate it as the Internal Energy of the plant. Automatic movements in plants are thus only exhibited under favourable tonic conditions. It has been shown that the plant displays rhythmic activity when subjected to different forms of constant stimulus, and we have now investigated separately the effects of such constant stimuli—chemical, electrical, thermal, photic, and hydrostatic. As has been explained before, if a given stimulus be not of sufficient intensity to evoke visible response, yet the absorbed energy may render the tissue capable of responding to another subsequent stimulus, which by itself would have been in- effectual ; in other words, the tissue is made excitable by the presence of a stimulus which has not of itself been adequate to cause response. When this latent excitability exceeds a certain amount, any further increase may be expressed in a visible manner by mechanical pulsation. Now, taking the various forms of stimulus to which the plant is constantly exposed—namely, warmth, light, moisture, and the action of the various chemical reagents, organic and inorganic, present in it or absorbed by it—it is clear that each of these exerts its stimulating effect independently ; any one by itself may then make the tissue excitable to the verge of response; in this condition, though there is no outward sign of the fact, there is a considerable amount of latent energy ; and the incidence of any second and additional form of stimulation is now sufficient to precipitate the excitation in visible form. These considerations will show how, by the cumulative and additive effects of all the forms of constant stimulation mentioned above, the plant may become so highly excitable as to manifest the fact by giving rise to responses which appear as automatic. The tonic condition is thus the latent excitatory INQUIRY INTO CAUSES OF AUTONOMOUS MOVEMENTS 309 condition, which is determined by the sum total of all these exciting factors. In connection with these facts, it is well to bear in mind that the excitability imparted by a stimulus does not always increase continuously with the intensity. On the contrary, there may be an optimum intensity beyond which excitability may be diminished. We have now seen that the energy which expresses itself in pulsatory movements may be derived by the plant, either directly from immediate external sources ; or from the excess of such energy, already accumulated and held latent in the tissue, aided by the incidence of external stimulation ; or from an excessive accumulation of such latent energy alone. In the last case, however, if the plant were kept isolated from all external supply of energy, it is clear that its reserve would become exhausted, and its automatic movements would cease. I have already described an experiment in which this arrest of pulsation took place in the case of a specimen of Desmodium kept ina dark room. We then saw that revival of response was only brought about when fresh stimulus was applied. And we have also seen the converse, namely, the ordinarily responding Szophytum, when supplied with excess of energy, become automatically responding. Cause of rhythmicity.— Having, then, seen that it is a constant source of energy, external or internal—the latter being really derived from a previous absorption of external stimulus—which maintains the so-called automatic movements in plants, we have still to determine how it is that a latent or constantly acting external stimulus can find only periodic expression? In connection with this we have seen how, when the minimal factor of stimulating intensity is exceeded, there is a manifestation of the fact by visible response. I have also shown (p. 245) that after each excitatory discharge there is a marked diminution of both conductivity and excitability ; and a new stimulus, or existing excess of stimulus, owing to the loss of these properties, is retarded for a time from producing any effect on the motile tissue. It 310 PLANT RESPONSE is only after the lapse of an interval that the protoplasm regains its original properties. There is thus an oscillatory variation of conductivity and excitability. It will therefore be seen how, under the circumstances, a constant stimulus, ora stimulus which is latent in the tissue, can find an excitatory expression only in a pulsating manner. Perhaps a physical model will enable us to visualise this process. Imagine a reservoir into which flows a constant supply of water. An elastic conducting pipe is led from the reservoir, and this pipe is constricted by a compressing spring. On the far open end of this pipe abuts the flattened end of an indicating lever. When water has been supplied for some time, its level is gradually raised, producing an increasing pressure. At a certain point, when the pressure becomes sufficiently great, the spring which keeps the elastic tube constricted gives way, and there is an impulsive discharge of water, which, impinging against the lever, gives rise to a visible response. But the yielding spring again closes, the tube is once more constricted, and thus by the oscillation of the spring which regulates the conduction of water, pulsating hydraulic impulses are kept up. On account of the oscillating mechanism, the outflow, and consequent mechanical response, are periodic, though the supply is constant. In this model the first period, before the pressure of water becomes sufficient to force open the spring, corresponds to the latent period in plant response; the oscillation of the flow of water corresponds to the oscillation of conductivity ; and the responding lever corresponds to the motile leaflet. Similarly, an ill-fitting spring tap is thus often thrown into a pulsating movement, by the constant pressure of water from the main, and there is then seen a rhythmic play of the water-jet. ‘ After-effect’ and its relative persistence.—In the ex- periment on Bzophytum under the continued action of light, we saw that for a period of one minute, during which the plant was absorbing light, there was noresponse. Then after this latent period of one minute, the energy absorbed reached the verge of response. The excitatory discharge was followed by a INQUIRY INTO CAUSES OF AUTONOMOUS MOVEMENTS 3II retardation of conductivity and excitability, which was, how- ever, gradually recovered, and there were produced a second and then successive periodic responsive movements. In the hydraulic model, if the capacity of the reservoir be small, the cessation of the water-supply will cause an immediate cessation of the rhythmic movement of the indicating lever. In Bzophytum, similarly, we found that the periodic response continued as long as energy was supplied, and that the movement soon stopped on the stoppage of the supply of external energy. But if the capacity of the reservoir be great, the accumulation may be sufficient to maintain the oscillation for a considerable length of time, even after the main supply is cut off. And we see that in Desmodium the responsive movements continue in a persistent manner, though the immediate source of stimulation be interrupted. A similar difference in the persistence of after-effects, depending on relative capacities for storage of energy, is seen in the two classes of inorganic substances, which are distinguished as fluorescent and phosphorescent. In the first, the responsive emission of light caused by a preceding excitation is extremely short-lived; but in the latter it continues long after the light stimulus has ceased to act. Thus Desmodium may continue to exhibit rhythmic movements, though not at the moment exposed to the marked action of any special source of stimulus. But for the display of long-continued rhythmic movements it should previously have absorbed a considerable amount of energy from an external source—in other words, it must have been exposed to those circum- stances which produce a favourable tonic condition. We have thus obtained some insight into that very ob- scure phenomenon which is known as the after-effect. By the inertia of the organism there is a certain loss of time before response begins to take place, and this determines the latent period. But when the stimulus has already initiated movement, the responding organ will, through the same inertia, continue to show this movement even when the stimulus has ceased to act. There is another factor, however, which 312 PLANT RESPONSE determines the persistence of this after-effect, namely, the larger or smaller capacity of the tissue itself to hold energy latent within it. In the attempt to investigate the cause of automatic movements in Desmodium, there were three points of inquiry which had to be determined. First, there was the question of the seat of excitation; second, that of the nature of the stimulus which maintained the rhythmic pulsation ; and third, the determination of the process by which constant stimulus found periodic expression. I have shown that the seat of excitation in Desmodium is neither central nor peripheral, but is in, or in immediate con- tiguity with, the motile tissue itself, which resembles in this respect the animal heart, the seat of excitation there also being in the cardiac tissue itself (Chapter XXVI.). I have also shown that as the cause of excitation it is not necessary to have any sudden varzation. A constant stimulus, of whatever nature, is found efficient to produce excitation. I have also demonstrated that periodic pulsation is pro- duced in Desmodium at standstill, by a constant thermal stimu- lus ; also that periodic pulsations are produced in Azophytum and in Desmodium by the constant action of a chemical stimulant. I have shown further that rhythmic excitation was produced in Averrhoa carambola by the passage of a constant electric current. And, finally, I have shown that, just as in the retina, under constant stimulus of light we have periodic visual excitations, so also in Azophytum, under con- stant stimulus of light we obtain periodic excitations which give rise to rhythmic movements. In the animal tissue, similar multiple rhythmic responses are met with under constant stimulus. Since we have found it to be a fundamental characteristic of the tissue of a rhythmically-responding plant like zo- thytum or Desmodium to give a response which cannot be increased by any excess in the stimulus-intensity (maximal response or none), we should expect that the excess over the INQUIRY INTO CAUSES OF AUTONOMOUS MOVEMENTS 313 minimally effective stimulus must remain latent in the tissue. This is evidenced by the fact that on applying a very strong stimulus we obtain, not a single but multiple responses. Thus all the various forms of constant stimulus to which it is exposed—warmth, light, moisture, and the different chemical reagents, organic and inorganic, present in or absorbed by it—become latent, and the sum total of all these stimulating factors determines its ‘tonic’ condition. When this accumulated latent energy exceeds a certain value, it is visibly manifested in the form of the so-called ‘automatic ’ movements. The perzodicity of the excitatory discharges which give rise to rhythmic movements in a plant that is under constant latent stimulus, is brought about by the peculiarity which has been demonstrated, that after each discharge the con- ductivity and excitability of the tissue are diminished, and are only gradually regained. This oscillation in the conduc- tivity regulates the outflow of energy, and causes the rhythmicity of the responsive movements. SUMMARY In Desmodium, the seat of excitation of the lateral leaflets lies in the motile organ itself. Multiple response is produced in the plant, as in the ani- mal, under constant thermal, chemical, or electrical stimulus. The retina, under constant stimulus of light, exhibits periodic visual pulsations. Similarly, the leaflet of Bzophytum, and that of Desmodium at standstill, under continuous stimulation of light, exhibit rhythmic mechanical pulsations. Liophytum, when raised to a temperature of about 29° C., becomes automatically responding. Under these circum- stances, the only difference between the so-called automatism of Bzophytum and Desmodium is that, in the latter case, the critical thermo-tonic condition is arrived at about 12° C. earlier. 314 PLANT RESPONSE The energy which expresses itself in pulsatory movements may be derived by the plant, either directly from immediate external sources, or from the excess of such energy, already accumulated and held latent in the tissue, aided by the incidence of external stimulation, or from an _ excessive accumulation of such latent energy alone. By ‘tonic’ condition is meant the latent excitatory con- dition of the plant, as determined by the sum total of the stimulating factors which are, or have been, derived from its environment. In other words, the tonic condition depends on the internal energy of the plant. In rhythmic tissues, a constant stimulus, external or internal, finds pulsatory expression in consequence of the oscillatory variation of conductivity and excitability. The duration of rhythmic movements, in the absence of any external exciting cause, depends on the amount of energy previously absorbed and held latent in the plant. The persistence of this after-effect, therefore, depends also on the greater or less capacity of the tissue for storage of energy. These rhythmic movements thus appear to be automatic, but when the reserve is exhausted, the so-called automatic move- ments come to a stop. Renewal of pulsatory movements can then take place only on the supply of fresh energy from without. CHAP PER: XV INFLUENCE OF VARIOUS CHEMICAL REAGENTS ON THE AUTONOMOUS RESPONSE OF DESMODIUM GYRANS The recorder and experimental chamber—Absolute measurement of period and amplitude of Desmodium-oscillation—Responsive significance of up and down movements deduced from (a) analogy with response of Afzmosa; (6) test of increased internal hydrostatic pressure—‘ Systolic’ contraction and ‘ diastolic’ expansion of Desmodium pulvinus—Mode of application of chemical reagents — Action of chemical reagents modified by : tonic condition of plants ; strength of solution; and duration of application—Effect of anzesthetics—Effect of alcohol—Effect of carbonic acid—Effects of ammonia and of carbon disul- phide—Effect of copper sulphate solution, either when applied externally, direct on the pulvinus, or internally—Spark-record of Desmodium-pulsation. HAVING thus, in the last chapter, traced the causes of autonomous movements in plants, I shall now, taking Desmo- dium gyrans as the type, describe the effect of various agencies on the so-called ‘spontaneous’ responses of its lateral leaflets. With regard to the experimental arrangements for making the record, I have already in Chapter I. described how records may be obtained, using the intact plant for experiment. The automatic movements of the leaflets, how- ever, persist, even after the petiole bearing them is detached, the cut end being kept in water. Under proper conditions, the rhythmic pulsations of the detached specimen will continue for acouple of days. In order, therefore, to subject the motile organ to various modifying conditions, it is much more con- venient to use such a specimen than the whole plant. The recorder and experimental chamber.—As the extent of the movement of the tip of the leaflet is consider- able, no magnification is necessary for the record. A single cocoon thread is attached to the middle of the leaflet by a 316 PLANT RESPONSE drop of shellac varnish, the other end of the thread being tied to the longer arm of the Optic Lever, which is 30 cm. long. The distance of the recording drum from the mirror is also 30 cm. The length of the lever arm being thus equal to the distance of the drum, and the extent of the angular move- ment being by reflection doubled, it will be seen that the movement is magnified twice. The thread is attached, however, to the middle of the leaflet, and the record there- Fic. 125. Experimental Apparatus for Making Records of Pulsation of Desmodium Leaflet, P, mounted in U-tube in plant chamber, and attached to long arm of Optical Lever, L ; M, mirror attached to fulcrum-rod ; b, recording drum ; I, 0, inlet and outlet pipes for gases and vapours introduced into plant chamber ; C, electric heating coil. fore gives the movement of the tip unmagnified. The records given in some of the figures are thus without magnification. In others, again, the records are on a reduced scale. It will be understood here that the extent of movement will vary with the length of the leaflet, some of these being very small, and others relatively large. In the apparatus shown in fig. 125, the smaller chamber contains the motile leaflets mounted in a tube filled with water. By means of an inlet-pipe, different gases may be CHEMICAL REAGENTS ON AUTONOMOUS PULSATION 317 introduced into the chamber for any desired length of time, after which fresh air may be re-introduced. In order to produce variations of temperature there is a coil of wire for electric heating. The automatic movements of the leaflet, both up and down, take place in some cases as a number of jerks, which may pass gradually into continuous movement. In others they are continuous from the beginning. From the normal or highest position, the leaflet, generally speaking, sinks some- whatrapidly. Having reached its maximum depressed position, there is a pause, after which there is rather a slow return to its original position. The up and down motion is in some cases approximately straight. In other cases, the sub-petiole of the leaflet is slightly twisted after its descent, and the curve described becomes more circular. For the purpose of the present investigation, specimens were selected in which the movement of the leaflet took place gradually, without jerks, the up and down move- ments being approximately in a straight line. Absolute measurement of period and amplitude of Des- modium - oscillation. — The period of a single oscillation varies with the temperature and the tonic condition of the plant. In winter it may be as long as five, in summer as short as two minutes, and when the tempera- ture is artificially raised, the period may be even further re- Fic. 126. Photographic Record of Pulsation of Desmodium The up movement of the record duced to one minute. Asacon- means down movement of the ; - leaflet in this and all subsequent crete example, affording a clear Fanos! idea of the general characteristics of the pulsatory movement of Desmodium, 1 shall give the following results, obtained from a photographic record which gives the extent of the absolute movement (fig. 126). 318 PLANT RESPONSE The period of the complete vibration was in this case 35 minutes, of which the down movement was accom- plished in the course of I°5 minute. The up movement was relatively slower, and was accomplished in two minutes. The mean amplitude of pulsation—that is to say, the vertical distance travelled by the tip of the leaf—was 25 mm. The fact that the down movement is, generally speaking, relatively the quicker is seen visually demonstrated in the photographic record of uniform pulsations obtained with another specimen (fig. 127). In connection with this, certain pecu- liarities of photographic action should be borne in mind. It is found that a short exposure gives an image which in the case of a line is ayeneiy ey ; i very thin and sometimes y \ \ \ y \ \ yy consists of only the . faintest impression; but Fic, 127. Photographic Record of Uniform when the exposure is Pulsations in Desmodiune , e prolonged the line is much thickened. Hence, in the records, the faint or more sharply outlined portions indicate responsive down movements which were relatively rapid. In fig. 127, therefore, these differences of line afford us a graphic representation of the various rates of movement, and durations of pause, in the different parts of thecurve. In all the photographic records given here and elsewhere, we are thus able to distinguish the down movements by the relative thinness of the recording line. In fig. 134, at the end of this chapter, will be found a spark-record of the pulsation of Desmodium in which the different rates of move- meager A Aah Period of each’pulsation = 2°7 minutes. ment at. different stages of the response can be distinguished at a glance, In the multiple responses of Pzophytum, though, generally J | - ) o~ ys @ 7 CHEMICAL REAGENTS ON AUTONOMOUS PULSATION 319 speaking, the down movement is more rapid, yet at times the up and down inovements approximate to each other in this respect. I have occasionally found similar instances in the pulsatory movement of Desmodium, when there would be only a slight difference between the rates of the up and down movements. The normal rates again may even become reversed under the influence of external agencies. Responsive significance of up and down movements.— A question of some difficulty and importance arises here as to the significance, whether of contraction or relaxation, of these up and down movements. We saw in the cases of Mimosa and Biophytum that the down movement was due to the relatively greater contraction of the lower half of the pulvinus, and the up movement to recovery or relaxation. It must be borne in mind that the upper half of the pulvinus of Mimosa, like the lower, is also excitable, though in a minor degree, as will be shown in a subsequent chapter. We have further seen that the more excitable half responds earlier to stimulus. The depression of the leaf, then, is due to the earlier and greater contraction of the lower half of the organ ; and its subsequent erection, to a natural expansive recovery, possibly aided by the later and feebler contraction of the upper half of the pulvinus. Arguing from analogy, we may regard the movement of the Desmodzum \eaflet as essentially similar to this. For here too we find, generally speaking, that the down movement is the more energetic, and the up movement relatively the slower, of the two. Hence we may infer that in Desmodium the down movement is due to contraction and the up movement to relaxation. Test by increased internal hydrostatic pressure.—It will be shown on page 349 that when Desmodium in a sub- tonic condition undergoes arrest of pulsation, an increase of internal hydrostatic pressure is found to renew the rhythmic activity. With fairly high internal pressure, the frequency of the pulsation is increased, though the amplitude is decreased. In order now to test the responsive significance of the up and down movements respectively, I tried the effect of an increase 320 PLANT RESPONSE of internal hydrostatic pressure in shifting the vibration-limits of the Desmodium \eaflet. It will be remembered that in the case of MWzmosa the effect of this increased internal hydrostatic pressure was to If, then, the same result should follow in the case of Desmodium, we should be justified in inferring that the mechanics of the motile organ were similar in the two cases. The increase of in- ternal hydrostatic pressure was in this case effected by the same method as was employed in that of JMZzmosa—that is to say, the cut end of the petiole of Des- modium was placed in one limb of a U-tube, filled with water, and the pressure was increased by adding water, so as to raise the level at the free end of the tube. As the leaflet of Desmodium, how- cause the erection of the leaf above its normal position. Fic. 128. Displacement of Mean Position of Vibration of Des- modium Leaflet by Increased U, Internal Hydrostatic Pressure upper limit ; L, lower limit ; M, mean position under normal conditions ; U’, L’, M’, corre- sponding positions under in- creased internal hydrostatic pressure ; MM’ is the extent of displacement upwards. In- creased internal pressure, gene- ever, is in constant oscillation, we must regard the mean of its vibration-limits as the normal mean, and the extent of the leaflet’s erection under increase of hydrostatic pressure must be rally speaking, produces a movement towards expansion, and tends to diminish the amplitude of the pulse, while increasing the frequency. measured by the shifting upwards of this mean. In carrying ment, I found, out this experi- on applying the increase of internal pressure exerted by a water-column of 10 cm., that, as will be seen in fig. 128, the lower limit upwards in the record by ris The mean position was thus raised by 7°5 mm. As the magnification of the record was in this particular case five times, it will be seen that the pressure exerted by a height of 10 cm. of of oscillation was displaced mm. and the upper limit by 3°5 mm. CHEMICAL REAGENTS ON AUTONOMOUS PULSATION 321 water had thus produced an absolute displacement of the mean position of the leaflet, of 1:5 mm. Considering these facts, it becomes reasonable to regard the motile indications of Desmodium as similar to those of Mzmosa; hence the down position of the Desmodium leaflet may be regarded as one of contraction, the up position being one of relaxation. Thus in Desmodium the down position of the leaflet corre- sponds to the systolic contraction, and the up position to the diastolic expansion of the heart. Mode of application of chemical reagents.—Having now to some extent determined the character of the move- ments of the Dessmodium leaflet, we shall proceed to observe in detail the modification of their movements by the action of various chemical re- agents. Three different methods of application may be employed. In the first place, the chemi- cal reagent may be dis- solved in the water in which the specimen is f : FIG. 120. Method of Application of placed. The solution will Chemical Agent to Cut End of Petiole thus reach the motile organ by the same process as that which brings about the ascent of sap. The characteristic action of the chemical reagent will be demonstrated in the subsequent modification of the responses. This method of applying a reagent at one point—in this instance the cut end of the petiole—and observing the sub- sequent physiological effect on the distant motile organ, is of special interest and importance in the case of poisons. For it serves to elucidate the obscure question of the ascent of sap through tissues that have been killed by poison (p. 385). In order to observe and compare the responses of the - 322 PLANT RESPONSE given plant, before and after the application of the solution, it is necessary that the continuity of the record should be in no way disturbed. For this purpose I insert the specimen in the arrangement shown in the diagram (fig. 129). One end of the tube is connected with a funnel, F, by means of an india-rubber tubing ; the other end is provided with a stop- cock. The tube is first filled with water, and the stop-cock, s,closed. The normal responses of the leaflet are now taken, with the petiole in water. Next, by proper manipulation of the stop-cock, the water is allowed to run out, and its place is taken by the chemical solution which is passed in at the funnel. The record which is now taken exhibits the effect of the drug. The second of our three methods of experiment is that of direct application—that is to say, touching the motile organ itself with a drop of the solution. In this case, the modifi- cation of response takes place rapidly. And, lastly, gaseous substances may be introduced or withdrawn from the plant chamber, by means of suitable inlet and outlet pipes. Action of chemical reagents modified by: tonic con- dition of tissue; strength of solution; duration of appli- cation.—There are certain general considerations of a very significant kind which it will be well to specify at this point. _ Though the fundamental effect of any given reagent is definite, yet certain minor variations of this effect, due to the constitution of the individual plant, are liable to occur, and these are often extremely suggestive. Two successive experiments, for example, were performed to determine the action of the poison mercuric chloride on different specimens ; one of these was extremely vigorous, and the other the reverse. The effect of the poison on the robust specimen consisted in depressing the pulsation, and reached its maximum some fifteen minutes after application. But subsequently the plant appeared to shake off the influence of the poison, and the pulses slowly recovered, till in the course of an hour they had once more become normal. The effect on the weakly specimen, however, was somewhat different. Instead of depression, the immediate result of application was a transi- CHEMICAL REAGENTS ON AUTONOMOUS PULSATION 323 tory exaltation of the amplitude of oscillation, which was doubled, though at the same time the rhythm became slowed. After ten minutes, however, these pulsatory movements grew irregular and depressed, and the plant succumbed to the action of the toxic agent, its pulsation undergoing complete arrest forty-five minutes after application. In the case of a weakly specimen, again, on filling the chamber with an atmosphere of carbonic acid gas, the pulsations soon come to a stop, and unless fresh air be quickly introduced, this arrest becomes permanent. Butwith a vigorous specimen, the depres- sion produced by this gas is very slow, and the permanent arrest does not take place till after a considerable lapse of time. The effect of an agent, again, depends on the strength of the solution, and the duration of application. A solution which, in larger quantities, would produce depression, will often, if given in very diluted form, cause the exaltation of response. The sudden introduction of an agent which may ultimately produce arrest, may act as a transitory stimulus, bringing about a preliminary augmentation of response, to be followed later by depression and arrest. The application of a deleterious substance, again, for a short time, will cause a temporary depression, from which there is revival ; but too long-continued action of the same reagent will cause permanent arrest. Besides all these, there is the interesting phenomenon of accommodation, by which the plant becomes gradually accustomed to the action of any adverse circum- stance, and is thus rendered immune. Effect of anzsthetics.—-Taking a specimen of Desmo- dium, 1 passed ether-vapour into the plant chamber. The pulsatory movement which had hitherto been fairly uniform now showed a transitory exaltation, and then fell with remarkable regularity of decrement. The response next showed an equally regular tendency towards the gradual recovery of its previous amplitude, but with longer period, this being protracted, from the normal two to four minutes. Finally, the pulsation of this specimen was abolished, the leaflet remaining in a position of relative relaxation, thirty Y2 324 PLANT RESPONSE minutes after the first introduction of ether into the plant chamber. On blowing off the ether-vapour, there was in this case no revival of response. In those cases, however, in which the ether- vapour is more diluted with air, or applied for a shorter period, the depression is | temporary only, and is fol- 1 Wed] bee lowed by revival. But if bi ay Lgbh /\ L a larger quantity of ether- vapour be at once introduced, a permanent arrest, in the relaxed position, quickly en- Fic. 130. Photographic Record of ; Effect of Ether Vapour, Large Dose sues. Before this happens, Arrow marks moment of application. there may be one or two Putationareced in wporreexel, spasmodic flutterings, but that the up movement of the record these quickly subside, as will or Aer to a down movement he seen in the photographic record given (fig. 130). Effect of alcohol.—The effect of this reagent is much modified by the tonic condition of the specimen. For example, in the case of a weaklier plant, the application, even of dilute solutions, induces rapid diminution and arrest of pulsation. More vigorous specimens can, however, with- stand the deleterious effect of this drug, and bear stronger doses. In the photographic record here given (fig. 131) a 5 per cent. solution is seen to induce a greater regularity 5 and amplitude of pulsation. The subsequent application ofa 10 per cent. solution causes a moderate, and 15 per cent.a still greater, depression. The application of a 5 per cent. solution to a weakly specimen, however, is found, as said before, to induce a depression so great as to cause a speedy arrest. Effect of carbonic acid.—The first effect of the intro- duction of this gas is sometimes one of exaltation. This is however, brief, and is followed by depression and sudden permanent arrest of pulsation in weaklier specimens. In CHEMICAL REAGENTS ON AUTONOMOUS PULSATION 325 other cases, again, an earlier re-introduction of fresh air into the chamber is sufficient to restore the specimen to its FIG, 131. Photographic Record of Effect of Alcohol Pulsations to left show normal response. ‘ Marks application of 5 per cent. solution ; ¢’ application of Io per cent. ; 4’' application of 15 per cent. natural pulsatory activity. I give here (fig, 132) a photo- graphic record of the effect of this gas on a vigorous speci- men, in which its action is seen to be somewhat gradual. One curious phe- nomenon which I have no- ticed in connection with the effect of this gas, is that when it remains stag- Banesin the.chamber its depressing effect is much more rapid than when a current is allowed to stream through. In the rnanner which [| FIG. 132. Photographic Record of Effect of Carbonic Acid Gas Arrow marks moment of application. have just described, I have carried out further a number of experiments on the action of various gases and vapours, such as ammonia, carbon disulphide, and others, all of which 326 PLANT RESPONSE are found to cause an arrest of the rhythmic movements of Desmodium. It is not necessary to go into these in detail, the experiments already given affording sufficient informa- tion for their successful repetition. I will only mention here the very interesting and impor- tant fact, that I find acids and alkalies, generally speaking, to produce effects which are in a certain physiological sense antagonistic. These effects, together with the influence of temperature on rhythm, and the action of tetanising electric shocks on the autonomous movements of Desmodium, will be found fully described in Chapters XX VI. and XXVII., where the remarkable parallelism of their influence on rhythmic animal and vegetable tissues will be demonstrated. I shall con- clude the present chapter by describing the action of a strongly poisonous reagent on the pulsatory movements of Desmodium. Effect of copper sulphate solution.—I carried out two experiments on similar specimens to test the effect of this reagent. In the first, the solution was applied directly on the pulvinus, and produced a very quick arrest of its rhythmic activity (fig. 133). In the second case, the ap- plication was made at the cut end of the petiole, as already de- scribed, which was at a distance of 2 cm. from FIG. 133. Photographic Record of Effect of Copper Sulphate Solution Applied on the the pulvinus. In this Pulvinus case the arrest took place much later, that isto say, thirty minutes after the application. This delay was due to the fact that the poisonous solution had to ascend the intervening distance before it could affect the rhythmic activity of the tissue at the pulvinus. This experiment will Arrow marks moment of application. CHEMICAL REAGENTS ON AUTONOMOUS PULSATION 327 be found important, as touching a later investigation on the ascent of sap. It is to be noticed that the petiole allows the poisonous solution which kills it to pass upwards through it. Spark-record of pulsation of Desmodium.—Before ending this chapter, I shall give a spark-record of a single pulsation in a leaflet of Desmodium. The successive sparks were produced at intervals of 5 seconds, and a glance at the record affords a visual demonstration of the peculiar characteristics of the move- ment of the leaflet. The up line as usual indicates down movement. It is thus seen that, after a pause in the highest position,asudden jig, 134. Spark-record of Single Pul- excitatory impulse is de- sation in Leaflet of Desmodiune veloped, which is eradually Interval between successive sparks = 5 seconds. exhausted, as the lowest position is reached. The up movement takes place more cradually,and at a much slower rate. The results are shown in the following table. TABLE SHOWING RATES OF MOVEMENT AT DIFFERENT STAGES OF PULSATION IN DESMODIUM. Down movement Up movement 45 seconds Total period 70 seconds | Average rate . ‘*61 mm. per sec. | Average rate : ‘4 mm. per sec. | Maximum rate . <7 4, 5, 5. |-Maximum rate. OI on... ho = OB | Duration of pause 40 seconds | Duration of pause 35 seconds | Total period SUMMARY The effect of a chemical reagent on a plant is modified to some extent by the tonic condition of the tissue. A vigorous plant will, generally speaking, withstand for a considerable time the action of deleterious agents; a weakly specimen succumbs more quickly. 328 PLANT RESPONSE The effect of a reagent depends also on the strength of the solution. A reagent which, in strong solution, induces depression, may, if given in small quantities, cause exalta- tion. The effect of a reagent depends also on the duration of application. The temporary depression produced by a short application is overcome by the self-accommodation of the plant. But it will succumb to too long or too strong an application of the same reagent. The effects of the various reagents on autonomous response are, generally speaking, similar to their effects on simple response. The depressed position of the leaflet of Desmodium represents a ‘systolic’ contraction, and the up position a ‘diastolic’ relaxation, of the motile organ. Increased internal hydrostatic pressure increases the extent of the relaxed or ‘diastolic’ limit. The effect of too strong an application of ether is to abolish response, the arrest of pulsation usually taking place. in a relaxed position. The immediate effect of application is generally a transient exaltation of response. The effect of vapour of alcohol is usually a transient exaltation. If the application be prolonged, the result is a permanent arrest of pulsation. Carbonic acid sometimes produces a transient exaltation of response, and always a subsequent depression, which, under the long-continued action of this gas, may pass into permanent arrest. Copper sulphate solution, when applied directly on the pulvinus, quickly causes arrest of pulsation ; but if the cut petiole be allowed to absorb the solution, the final arrest does not take place till after the lapse of a certain period, required for the solution to ascend to the motile organ. CHAPTER. XXVI EFFECTS OF TEMPERATURE ON AUTONOMOUS RESPONSES Increase of frequency and diminution of amplitude of pulsation with rising temperature —Converse effect of fall of temperature—Similar effect in cardiac pulsation—Effect of the reduction of temperature to the thermo-tonic minimum —Explanation of diminution of amplitude of pulsation with rise of temperature —Anomalous use of the word ‘relaxation ’—Simple verses additive character of individual pulsation. IT is my intention in the course of the next chapter to make a comprehensive review of the similarities in all their characteristics of rhythmically responding tissues, both vege- table and animal. In the present chapter, then, we shall confine our attention to a study in detail of the influence of temperature in modifying the amplitude and period of rhythmic autonomous responses, exemplified in the case of plant-tissues by Desmodium and in that of the animal by cardiac muscle. As then we are about to study the effect of temperature on the period and amplitude of vibration of the Desmodium leaflet, it is clear that our first difficulty must be the securing of a specimen in which both are, to begin with, more or less uniform; for the pulsation of Desmodium, like that of the isolated frog’s heart used for experiments, is often irregular. It is thus only by careful selection that one can obtain suitable experimental subjects. A moderate increase of the internal hydrostatic pressure, however, will often have the effect of rendering the responses sufficiently uniform. Regulation of temperature.—The second difficulty in this investigation lies in subjecting the plant to the required rise or fall of temperature. A rise of temperature may be secured by any one of three different methods. (1) A spirit 30 PLANT RESPONSE ioe) flame may be applied underneath a bath of water in which the leaflets are placed. The temperature is thus gradually and continuously raised, and the successive pulsations, corresponding to different temperatures, are recorded in the usual manner by means of the Optic Lever. (2) Water at the required temperature may be syphoned into the bath, and the responses taken in the ordinary manner. (3) The air chamber in which the specimen is placed may be subjected to electric heating. The use of temperature may now be very accurately regulated by adjustment of the current, and records of pulsations may be taken at different and deter- minate temperatures. This last is the most perfect method, the two former, dependent as they are on the immersion of the specimen in a bath of water, having as compared with it many disadvan- tages. For the natural freedom of movement of the leaflet is hampered by the water, and more troublesome still is the difficulty which at times arises from capillary action in the partially immersed cocoon-thread, by which the leaflet is attached to the Optic Lever outside the bath. But we have not the same perfect facilities for lowering temperature in a gradual and continuous manner, as for raising it. This may be accomplished, however, sufficiently well for our purposes: (1) by placing fragments of ice in the air chamber; or (2) the pulvinus of the leaflet may be touched with cold water which has been reduced to the required temperature by means of ice. I find, however, (3) that a much better method is that of placing in the air chamber a coil of thin-walled metallic tubing, preferably of highly conducting copper. When cooled brine is made to circulate through this coil, the temperature of the chamber is lowered, and by regulation of the flow, by means of stop- cocks, it is possible to produce an adjustment of cooling. Effect of temperatures maximum and minimum.— Autonomous vibrations come to a stop when the temperature is sufficiently lowered. The temperature minimum at which this occurs depends, as we should expect, on the nature of TEMPERATURE AND AUTONOMOUS RESPONSES 331 the specimen—-whether Desmodium, Biophytum, or cardiac muscle—and also, with similar specimens, on the tonic condi- tion. With regard to the first of these points, we have seen that the autonomous vibration of Azophytum comes to a stop below 29° C. The pulsation of Desmodium is said to be arrested at 22° C., but I find that this is a matter which is much modified by the tonic condition of the particular plant. With vigorous specimens I have seen that the vibra- tion. may persist even at so low a temperature as 17° C. The thermo-tonic minimum in Bzophytum and Desmodium thus shows a difference, as already said, of about 12° C. ; and in the case of the frog’s heart this is still lower, and is said to be about 0° C. When, again, the specimens are raised to a maximum temperature, the pulsations come to a stop. The tempera- ture at which this takes place depends in part on the condition of the specimen. For example, with the frog’s heart, this maximum is sometimes at a temperature so low as 38°C. Jn other cases, pulsation may be detected even at so high a temperature as 44°C. Similarly, in Desmodium, I have found that the maximum temperature at which arrest took place was sometimes as low as 35° C.; but in certain specimens it did not occur till 45° C. A plant may, again, be accustomed gradually to high temperatures, and under these circumstances the maximum may be raised as much as 3°C. or 4° C. higher. Effect of temperature on period and amplitude of response.__The most marked phenomenon of effect of temperature on automatic pulsations, whether animal or vegetable, lies, however, in the fact that the period and amplitude are both affected. When the temperature is lowered, the amplitude of pulsation is enhanced, while the frequency is diminished. Conversely, with rise of tempera- ture, the amplitude is diminished, and frequency augmented. This is true not only of the pulsations of the rhythmic tissue of Desmodium, but also of those of the animal heart. This is seen in the two following records, where the first 332 PLANT RESPONSE set of responses in each series gives pulsations at the temperature of the room, while the second set in each gives pulsations of greater amplitude and smaller frequency, due to the lowering of temperature by several degrees (figs. 135, 136). Conversely, as already said, when the temperature is raised, the frequency is increased and the amplitude decreased. This is seen in a general way in the following photographic record (fig. 137), which I took with Fic. 135. Photographic Records of Autonomous Pulsations in VDes- modium, showing Increase of Amplitude and Decrease of Frequency, with Lowering of Temperature The pulsations to the left were taken at the ordinary temperature of the room, 29° C. Those to the right were taken when the temperature had been lowered to 25° C. Desmodium, the temperature being continuously raised, from 30° C. to 39° C. It will be seen how progressive in character is the diminution of amplitude and increase of “frequency in these responses. In another set of experiments in which I took records of the responses of Desmodium at various definite ascending temperatures, I found that at 19° C. the period of a single oscillation was 4°3 minutes. At 22° C. this was reduced to TEMPERATURE AND AUTONOMOUS RESPONSES 333 3'2 minutes, or nearly to two-thirds of the period at 19° C. At 28° C. it was found to be again reduced to 2'1 minutes, or half. Butat 40°C. it was only 1°4 minute, or one-third. Thus, while in 4°3 minutes, at 19° C. there is only a single beat, there are two beats at 28° C. and three beats ato SC: in the same time. I give below (fig. 138) a record of these responses at various temperatures. The re- cord given afterwards Fic. 136. Effect of Lowering of Tempera- EG ‘ 2 iy ture in Producing Increase of Amplitude (fig. 139) Snows Ow and Decrease of Frequency in Pulsation similar is the effect of of Frog’s Heart temperature on the The pulsations to the left represent normal : pulsations at the temperature of the amplitude and rhythm room. Those to the right were taken of the pulsation of the at a temperature several degrees lower : P i (after Brodie). animal heart. When the temperature of Desmodium is raised above 40° C. there appears to be an arrest of pulsation. But this need not be Fic. 137. Photographic Record of Pulsations of Desmodium during Continuous Rise of Temperature regarded as due to heat-rigor. For in magnified records I have often noticed that here we may have very much 334 PLANT RESPONSE quicker pulsations, but of an amplitude so small as usually to pass undetected. It is only under prolonged exposure to this relatively high temperature, or after exposure to a temperature above 50° C., that true heat-rigor sets in. This maximum temperature varies in individual cases with the tonic condition of the plant. Effect of the | redueiea@er temperature to the thermo-tonic minimum.—It has been said that, generally speaking, the amplitude of response increases with the lowering of temperature. It is evident, how- ever, that this process must have a limit. For we know that pulsation vanishes at the thermo-tonic mini- mum ; before this reduction of ampli- tude to zero it is clear that there must be some point where the increase of amplitude due to continuous cooling Fic. 138. Recordof Pulsa- must undergo reversal, or diminution. tions of Desmodium at Ate : Different Temperatures And this is what we should theoreti- cally expect, for since it isthe absorbed thermal energy that maintains the pulsation, it follows that, when this is diminished below par, the vibrational energy Nu 13° 23° 33° lic. 139. Record of Pulsations of Frog’s Heart at Different Temperatures|(Pembrey and Phillips) should also undergo diminution. We had an illustration of this (p. 305), when the automatically vibrating Azophytum TEMPERATURE AND AUTONOMOUS RESPONSES 335 at 35° C. was allowed to descend to the thermo-tonic minimum. It wasin that case found that there was a regular diminution of amplitude, and the pulsation afterwards dis- appeared below 29° C. (fig. 124). I was next desirous of determining whether this theoretical inference could be verified in the case of Desmodium. For this purpose I rapidly cooled the plant, by means of the cooling coil, through which ice-cold brine was. passed, a Fic. 140. Effect of Cooling to Thermo-tonic Minimum on Pulsation of Desmodium The first two pulses to the left were taken at the normal temperature ox 29? C.; those to the right during continuous cooling. The first response of the latter series occurred at 22° C. ; the second at 20° C. ; and the third at 17° C. Note that while in normal responses there are no sub-pulses, these are seen with increasing distinctness as the pulsation becomes continuously slower. They first make their appear- ance during the up movement of the first pulsation under cooling. In the two successive pulsations they are seen more and more clearly in the down as well as the up movements. photographic record being taken of the pulsations all the time. It will be noticed that the first effect of cooling was the normal increase of amplitude and prolongation of period. This latter— which at the temperature of the room had had a value of three minutes—was now prolonged to nearly six minutes. But the most’ interesting fact was that, as the thermo-tonic minimum was approached, the amplitude was reduced (fig. 140). 336 PLANT RESPONSE In the next experiment cooling was produced more suddenly, by application of cold water at about 4° C. to the pulvinus. We observe in this case how the quick reduction to the thermo-tonic minimum reduced the ampli- tude till the pulsation had come to a stop. I then allowed the leaf to return gradually to the temperature of the room, and it is very interesting to note the effect of increasing Fic. 141. Effect of Rapid Cooling by Ice-cold Water Normal pulsations recorded to the left. Effect or application of ice-cold water is seen in the production of diminished amplitude and abolition of pulsation. Gradual return to the temperature of the room revives the pulsation in a staircase manner, the period remaining approximately constant. Note that cooling, in this and previous figure, displaced the pulsation in a downward or contracted direction. Gradual warming, conversely, is seen in this figure to produce the opposite displacement towards relaxation. absorption of thermal energy from its surroundings. The increased energy thus absorbed is seen to give rise to increased amplitude of oscillation, in a staircase manner, which gradually approaches the original pulsation (fig. 141). In both these figures it will be noticed that cooling dis- places the pulsation in a downward or contracted direction. And in the last series of fig. 141 we see that the raising of the TEMPERATURE AND AUTONOMOUS RESPONSES = 337 temperature displaces it upwards or towards relaxation. These facts are of great importance, and should be borne in mind in reference to the explanation of the cause of variation of amplitude and period, which I shall bring forward. Explanation of diminution of amplitude of pulsation with rise of temperature.—We have thus seen, as we should theoretically have expected, that with the increased absorp- tion of energy at a higher temperature, the amplitude of vibration is also increased. How is it, then, that with the still further increase of absorption of energy, at still higher temperatures, the amplitude should undergo a diminution? When approaching the maximum point, where heat-rigor takes place, we can understand that the excitability of the tissue would be very much decreased, with a consequent reduction of amplitude of pulsation. But at temperatures of 25° C. to 30° C. excitability of the tissue could not be diminished. Indeed, I shall in Chapter XXXIII. adduce considerations to show that it must, at that temperature, be highly excitable, and we should have expected that this would have produced, in addition to the increased energy, an augmentation of vibrational amplitude. But, instead of this, we obtain the curious result which has been described, of a diminution, in the cases both of cardiac muscle and of Desmodium. It might be suggested that if increased activity, due to rise of temperature, increased the frequency of vibration, then this fact would be sufficient in itself to account for a diminution of amplitude ; for in this case, less time being allowed for each single vibration, its extent must be cur- tailed. But this consideration alone would not explain all the facts of the case; for we have seen that on approaching the thermo-tonic minimum, though the frequency of vibra- tion is reduced, and the period very much extended, yet the amplitude is also at the same time decreased (fig. 140). We thus see that the question of internal energy is important in this connection. An increase of internal energy may be expressed, as I shall show, in two different ways ; either, that Z 338 PLANT RESPONSE is to say, by increase of amplitude or by increase of frequency of pulsation. Increased internal energy, shown by: (a) Jucrease of amplitude, pertod remaining constant.—And we shall, as the simpler of the two, consider that case in which the latent energy, or tonic condition of the plant, is below par, that is to say, the case in which it isnearthethermo-tonic minimum. The effect of increased absorption of energy with rising temperature would here be indicated by increasing amplitude of pulsation, the period remaining constant. Conversely the reduction of latent energy with falling temperature would be indicated, when the period is constant, by the fall of amplitude. This we find fully illustrated in records obtained with Azophytum and Desmodium. Inthe former, when nearing the thermo-tonic minimum, it is found that while the period remains approxi- mately constant, that is to say, two and a half minutes, the amplitude of pulsation falls from 8 divisions at 35° C. to 5:5 divisions at 32° C. (fig. 124). In Desmodium, again, we find a converse case. Here, while the plant is rising from the thermo-tonic minimum to the normal condition, the amplitude of pulsation is seen to increase progressively, while the period of 2'4 minutes remains constant (fig. 141). (0) Increase of frequency.—Vaking next the case of a plant in the ordinary tonic condition, we find that the increase of internal activity, due to the greater absorption of energy during a rise of temperature, is exhibited by a higher frequency of vibration. . The reason why, with this increase of frequency, there is a diminution of amplitude of pulsation, is now to be explained. We have seen that an increase of internal energy, as caused by rise of temperature, brings about an increase of turgor, and that this increased turgor hastens the process of recovery, and by acting antagonistically to the con- tractile phase of responses diminishes its amplitude. In- creased internal pressure also, generally speaking, increases the frequency of vibration. If, then, the rise of temperature increases the turgor of Desmodium, as we have found it to do TEMPERATURE AND AUTONOMOUS RESPONSES 339 in plants in general, then the difninution of the amplitude of its vibration with higher temperature is explained ; and | shall be able to adduce independent proof that this is actually the case in Desmodium, for we have seen that the external indication of internal increase of turgor is the expansion of the organ, which produces a movement upwards, the same as that of relaxation. We found in the last series of responses in fig. 141, moreover, that when the temperature was raised gradually from the thermo-tonic minimum, the leaflet was more and more erected, or ‘relaxed.’ Cooling, conversely, produced diminution of turgor, and an opposite movement in the direction of fall or contraction (figs. 140 and 141). The anomalous use of the word ‘ relaxation.’ We have seen that the motile organ of Dessmodzum, when anesthetised, is brought to a state of standstill in a position of relaxation. Its tonic condition, by virtue of which it exhibits contractile response, has thus been abolished. We may then regard ether as having brought about a loss of tone, or as having reduced the tissue to the a-tonic condition. . An apparently similar position of relaxation may, however, be attained by the active process of expansion, which is the result of an increase of internal turgor. It would thus appear that we are liable to form many wrong inferences, as to the tonic changes undergone by the organ, if we too hastily conclude that expansion is always caused by loss of tone. Simple versus additive character of individual pulsa- tions.—One question, regarding which opposite views have been put forward hitherto, is that of the simple or composite nature of the individual pulsations of cardiac muscle. The movement of systole may, for example, be regarded as con- sisting either of a single or of the additive effect of several constituent contractions. In the production of tetanus in the case of muscle and also in that of contractile vegetable tissue we have seen several individual contractions, when following each other with sufficient rapidity, become merged in one appa- rently continuous contraction (figs. 49, 50). When less rapid, however, they are individually distinguishable. Similarly, Z2 340 PLANT RESPONSE in the case of cardiac pulsation, it is possible that one appa- rently simple contraction may in reality consist of several, which are rendered indistinguishable by the great rapidity of their succession. Now, since the pulsatory response of Desmodium is in every way so similar to that of cardiac muscle, its analysis might be expected to throw much light on this question ; and this more especially since it possesses the added advantage that its pulsation is executed in a period about one hundred times as long as that of cardiac muscle. The constituent elements of each pulsation, if such exist, ought thus, owing to the relative slowness of the movement, to be much more easy of detection. Now, it is often seen in watching the pulsatory movements of Des- modium that they proceed some- what discontinuously, or by jerks. Under favourable circumstances, nevertheless, the movements of the leaflet up and down become apparently continuous. When such pulsations are recorded ona Fic. 142. Photographic Record of Pulsation of Desmodium, showing Sub-pulses during rather rapidly moving drum, the slower Up Movement, as discrete nature of the movement Nodules can be brought out more easily in the case of the up movement, which is relatively slow. This is shown in a very interesting manner in the accompanying photographic record (fig. 142), where the subsidiary pulsations make themselves visible as nodules; places where the move- ment is slow appear thicker, on account of the photographic irradiation-effect. During the course of the single up move- ment recorded in this photograph, we may count as many as twenty-five of these sub-pulses. It has been said that these subsidiary movements are more easily detected when the general movement is relatively slow ; and in connection with this, it is extremely interesting to note the record of pulsations TEMPERATURE AND AUTONOMOUS RESPONSES 341 ) seen in fig. 140, where successive pulsations of the same leaflet, at first apparently continuous, are made to exhibit the sub-pulses with growing distinctness, as the period becomes progressively slowed down by cooling. First we are enabled to observe the sub-pulses during the up movement, and afterwards during the down move- ment also. I give another photo- graphic record also (fig. 143), in which these subsidiary pulses are seen at both the ‘systole’ and ‘diastole’ (see also fig. 175). Arguing from analogy, therefore, it becomes highly probable that any single pulsation of the heart also may be made up of similar dis- crete and constituent elements. FIG. 143. Photographic Record of Cyclic Group- ings in Autonomous Pul- sations of Desmodium, showing Sub-pulses While dealing with this question, it must be borne in mind that in each case the contractile organ as a whole is made up of a mass of in- dividually contractile ele- ments, the sum of whose separate and additive ac- tions it is which is seen as a single contraction and expansion of the whole organ. Thus, in Desmo- dium for example, a single pulsation of the motile organ is made up of sub- sidiary pulselets. The unit-pulses, again, may themselves be grouped in : Fic. 144. Photographic Record of Auto- larger systems, either as nomous Pulsation of Desmodium, the alternate waxings and showing Hourly Period wanings seen in periodic [Speed of drum = 5 cm. in one hour. ] sroupings (figs. 143 and 149), or in those periodicities of the order of an hour or so, which the plant sometimes 342 PLANT RESPONSE exhibits, under the influence of periodic variations of tem- perature and other factors. Anextremely interesting example of such an hourly periodicity is given above (fig. 144), where the mean plane of vibration is itself seen to exhibit a periodic up-and-down oscillation. There is, again, the still larger periodicity of diurnal variation of day and night. We thus see how complex may be these wave-systems, in which, superposed over large waves, are smaller waves, and on the latter still smaller wavelets. SUMMARY In Desmodium, as in cardiac muscle, rise of temperature produces increased frequency with diminished amplitude of pulsation. This is true within a certain normal range of temperature. When the temperature of Desmodzum is reduced to a thermo-tonic minimum—which is about 17° C. but subject to certain individual variations—the amplitude of pulsation, owing to the loss of internal energy, is decreased till there is an-arrest. If now the temperature be gradually raised, the pulsations, owing to the absorption of energy, become again increased in a staircase manner, the period remaining approxi- mately constant. Under normal tonic conditions, the decrease of amplitude of pulsation with rising temperature—when this is not excessive—is not indicative of loss of excitability. It is due to the increase of internal energy, which hastens recovery and acts antagonistically to the responsive movement of contrac- tion. The same explanation is probably applicable to the diminished amplitude of pulsations with rise of temperature, observable in cardiac response. Rise of temperature, by increasing the internal energy and consequent turgor of the plant, causes expansion of the organ, thus bringing about a shifting of the pulsation towards ‘ diastole.’ A converse effect, or shifting towards ‘systole,’ is seen in Desmodium, as thesresult of cooling. TEMPERATURE AND AUTONOMOUS RESPONSES 343 The process of relaxation in the motile organ may be the result of entirely different causes. It may be a consequence of a loss of tone, such as results from narcotisation; or it may be brought about by excessive turgor, caused by increased internal energy. The apparently simple rhythmic pulsations of Desmodium can be analysed and shown to consist of subsidiary minor pulsations. The ordinary pulsations, again, may show hourly and daily periodicities. CHAPTER XXVII SIMILARITIES OF RHYTHMIC RESPONSE IN VEGETABLE AND ANIMAL TISSUES The similarities, in their fundamental characteristics, of rhythmic tissues, animal and vegetable : (1) In responses—(2) In possession of long refractory periods — (3) In incapability of tetanus—Theories regarding the causation of heart-beat —The similarities of rhythmic tissues, animal and vegetable, as seen in: (1) The effects of internal hydrostatic pressure—(2) The effects of variation of tem- perature—(3) The periodic groupings of response—(4) The effect of barium salt—(5) The antagonistic actions of acid and alkali—Identity of rhythmic phenomena in animal and vegetable tissues. I HAVE, in the course of the previous chapters, shown the remarkable general similarities, extending through numerous details, between the responses in animal and vegetable tissues. These similarities, however, become still more striking when we compare the special characteristics of those plant and animal tissues which exhibit the property of rhythmicity—that is to say, those tissues which, under the action of a single strong stimulus, give rise to a multiple and rhythmic series of responses, such responses, under favourable circumstances, passing into the so-called automatic movements. In animal tissues, such rhythmic movements may be observed in their perfection in the case of cardiac muscle. The isolated heart, when brought to a state of temporary standstill, will give, in answer to a single stimulus, a single response, or to a sufficiently strong stimulus a multiple series of rhythmic responses ; and under favourable circumstances it will give automatic responses for a considerable length of time. Similarly, the plant Azophytum, under exceptionally favourable circumstances, exhibits what are apparently auto- matic responses ; and again, under ordinary conditions it RHYTHMIC RESPONSE IN PLANT AND ANIMAL = 345 gives a single response to a single stimulus, or if the stimulus be sufficiently strong, a multiple series of rhythmic responses. In the plant Desmodium, under favourable tonic conditions we observe automatic movements ; but when under less favourable circumstances, as for instance owing to the unfavourable season, it is brought to a state of standstill, it gives a single response to a single stimulus. When the stimulus, however, is strong, we have seen that it gives rise to a multiple series of responses, in a manner precisely like that of Bzophytum under similar circumstances. It will thus be seen that Szophytum in its ordinary condition may be regarded as equivalent to Desmodzum in a state of standstill. Similarities, in their fundamental characteristics, of rhythmic tissues, animal and vegetable: (1) /x responses.— In the matter of response, we have found that the rhythmic automatic movements of cardiac muscle are repéated in Desmodium under ordinary tonic conditions, and in Bzophytum under exceptionally favourable circumstances. Ina state of standstill, all three give a single response to a single moderate stimulus, and a multiple series of rhythmic responses to a sufficiently strong stimulus. The following tabular statement exhibits this parallelism in a concise form : TABULAR STATEMENT SHOWING SIMILARITIES IN THE RESPONSES OF RHYTHMIC ANIMAL AND VEGETABLE TISSUES At standstill ==> = aS a a Under favourable = ; conditions Moderate stimulus Sufficiently SHLOne | stimulus | ¥: \- : . ea eee ae = Single stimulus : ee be 8 a per Automatic Vp : Single stimulus, multiple series of : Cardiac muscle e : : rhythmic single response rhythmic movements responses Desmodium. Do. Do. Do. | c ; _ | eae Biophytum .. Do. | Do. Do. 346 PLANT RESPONSE (2) In possession of long refractory period —lIn order to study in detail the characteristics of response in Desmodzum, I took a plant in which the leaflets had come to a state of natural standstill. To such a specimen I applied the stimulus of a condenser discharge; it was found, as stated already, that a rather high electromotive charge (twenty-four volts) was required to produce response. The first few responses were somewhat feeble, owing to the sluggish condition of the tissue ; they then increased in a ‘staircase’ manner till they became uniform, the period of a complete response being now about six minutes. From this point on, the responses were the maximal possible, and a higher E.M.F. produced no notice- able increase. The most characteristic feature of these responses was the possession of a long refractory period, which we have also found to be characteristic of the response of Biophytum. With this specimen of Desmodium 1 found that when a second stimulus was given after three minutes, there was no further response. But a stimulus given after three and a half minutes was effective. It may be mentioned here that the length of the refractory period varies somewhat with the condition of the tissue, being relatively longer when that is sluggish. (3) In tncapability of tetanus—The rhythmic tissue of Desmodium thus resembles cardiac tissue, in the possession F1G. 145. Record showing that Rhythmic Tissue of Desmodium is Incapable of being Tetanised After the first two pulsations, strong tetanising electric shocks were applied continuously. No tetanic effect was produced, but the pulsation became somewhat irregular. of a marked refractory period. There is again another interesting similarity. A rhythmically beating cardiac tissue cannot be thrown into tetanus by quickly recurring electric RHYTHMIC RESPONSE. IN PLANT AND ANIMAL 347 shocks. An automatically moving Desmodium leaflet is also incapable of being thrown into a state of tetanus (fig. 145). Rapidly succeeding shocks do not produce tetanic contraction, though some irregularity may occur in the pulsation; exces- sively strong shocks kill the plant, and the pulsation is then permanently arrested. Theories regarding the causation of heart-beat.— Having thus seen how similar are the phenomena of rhyth- micity in cardiac muscle and in plants, we may proceed to inquire into the theories which have been proposed to account for the automatic pulsation of the heart. It has been suggested : (1) That discrete impulses are sent out from certain motor nerve-centres in the heart to the muscular tissue, thus causing the periodic heart-beat. Assuming the correctness of this theory, however, the difficulty is merely transferred, for we have still to account for the rhythmic excitation of the nerve. But that the rhythmic heart-beat is not fundament- ally due to rhythmic impulses from nerve-centres, has been proved from facts discovered by various observers: (a) that the isolated ganglion-free apex of the frog’s heart may be thrown into rhythmic activity by stimulus; it has also been shown by Gaskell (4) that the apex of the tortoise-heart, which is free from nerve-cells, is capable of rhythmic move- ments ; and (c) it is found that even in the embryo, before any connection with the central nervous system has been established, there is a rhythmic heart-pulsation. (2) That cardiac muscle may have the inherent property of rhythmicity. This explanation, however, by itself, is incom- plete, for it takes no account of the stimulus which must exist, in order to give rise to rhythmic expression. (3) That the pulsation of the heart is maintained by some ‘inner stimuli, its rhythmicity being brought about by the long refractory period peculiar to cardiac muscle. Independent light, however, may be expected to be thrown on the question of the causation of spontaneous 348 PLANT RESPONSE rhythmic action in cardiac muscle, by the consideration of similar phenomena in plants, especially since it can be shown that their similarity is manifested under numerous varying conditions, and extends to fundamental characteristics. We have already seen that there is a similarity of funda- mental characteristics between the response of cardiac muscle and that of rhythmic vegetable tissue. . Similarities of rhythmic tissues, animal and vege- table.—We shall now, therefore, observe in detail those other and more special similarities which are exhibited in a com- mon modification of response under varying external condi- tions, by Desmodium and cardiac muscle alike, and I shall first describe the remarkable effect produced on both by internal pressure. (1) The effects of internal hydrostatic pressure.—lt is found that a heart which has come to a condition of standstill may be set into rhythmic activity by filling the cavity of the heart with liquid. Endo-cardiac pressure is thus found to act as a stimulus. In Desmodium, when the season is favourable, the tissue is in a turgid condition, and there is a considerable internal hydrostatic pressure, which we have seen to be advantageous to the maintenance of rhythm. This turgid condition depends on the ascent of sap, which, as I shall show in Chapter XXVIII., depends again on the rhythmic activity of certain tissues. Thus,in the summer season, we have the conditions most favourable for the maintenance of rhythmic activity in Desmodium. But with the approach of winter the vigour of the plant and its turgid condition undergo a marked decline, and the autonomous movement of the leaflet then comes to a stop. It appeared to me that this cessation of movement might to a great extent be due to the diminution of internal hydrostatic pressure, and I undertook experiments to see whether the pulsatory movement could be renewed by an increase of this pressure, just as increased endo-cardiac pres- sure was found to renew the beating of the heart. The RHYTHMIC RESPONSE IN PLANT AND ANIMAL 349 experiment was carried out by mounting a detached petiole containing the motile leaflets at one end of a limb of a U-tube. Any desired pressure could be ex- erted by varying the height of the second limb, which was con- nected with the first by india-rubber tubing. By exerting internal pres- sure in this manner, I was able to pro- duce vigorous rhythmic Fic. 146. Curve showing Relation between movements of leaflets Temperature and Period of Pulsation in which were, before this, DENT in an absolutely qui- Abscissa represents HOE | BoeehS and ordinate on time, in tenths of a minute. escent condition. The beneficial effect of this constant internal pressure was further seen demonstrated by the extreme regularity and persistency of the rhythmic beats. Even in the best season of the year, the pulsations are irregular, and come to an occasional stop ;_ but under the action of internal pressure, I have found the detached leaflet to main- tain its rhythmic activity unimpaired for nearly one hundred hours. In con- nection with this question of the increase of inter- Fic. 147. Curve showing Relation between Temperature and Period of nal pressure, it should be Pulsation in the Heart of a Frog mentioned here that, after Ordinate represents time, in tenths of a ) second. the normal condition of turgidity has been established, a further increase of internal pressure is found to increase the frequency of pulsation. PLANT RESPONSE ~~ Wt 12) Excessive pressure, however, brings on irregularity, or even stoppage, of autonomous movement. (2) The effects of variation of temperature—I have shown in the last chapter how perfectly similar are the effects of tem- perature in causing variations of the ampli- tude and frequency of pulsation in rhythmic tissues, both vegetable and animal. As regards the effect of temperature on the period, the similarity is strikingly exhibited in the two curves given above, showing the relation between temperature and perfod in Desmodzum and in cardiac muscle. It will be seen that in both (figs. 146 and 147) the fall of period with increase of temperature is at first rapid and then slow. 5 (3) Periodic groupings of response.— he Pru eae a Another very remarkable similarity be- Alternation of y A Deas in Des- — tween the pulsations of Desmodium and of cardiac muscle lies in their exhibition of periodic groupings, very simple types of which, given by Desmodium, are seen in the photographic records (figs. 148, 149). In fig. 146 we have an alternate waxing and waning of pulsation, and a remarkably similar record, given by frog’s WAVAVAUILAN Fic. 149. Periodic Groupings Fic. 150. Simple Alternation of Pulsation in Desmodium of Pulsation in Frog’s Heart (Pembrey and Phillips) heart, is seen in fig. 150. These groupings are of various degrees of complexity, one type of such heart-beats being that known as Luciani’s groups, where the successive groups are separated by a long pause. Now, similar groups with RHYTHMIC RESPONSE IN PLANT AND ANIMAL 351 intermediate pause are also seen in the pulsation of Des- modium. \ have again shown that periodic rhythms of various degrees of complexity are seen, not only in the pulsations of Desmodium, but also in the multiple responses of Azophytum, and even in the electrical responses in plants. (4) Effects of barium salt——There are agencies, such as internal pressure and certain chemical reagents, which induce regularity of pulsation in irregularly pulsating rhyth- mic tissues. Conversely, there are others, which pro- duce the opposite effect, that is to say, a regular pulsation, after such an application be- comes irregular. When the heart pulsations Fic. alae a SeeoE Chloride On ON L/@S/720A2UI2 are regular, it is found that The regular pulsation, seen to the left, addition of Veratrin disturbs becomes irregular after application 4 : of this reagent. the uniformity. Remember- ing the general similarity of the action of Vevatrzn and barium salts, I applied a 5 per cent. solution of this substance to the pulvinus of Desmodium leaflet, which was executing very regular vibrations. The application of this reagent disturbed the regularity -of the pulsation, and somewhat irregular = 10 ; 4 iY \J Fic. 152. Arrest of Beat of Ventricle of Frog at Diastole by Application of Acid at Arrow Record to be read from right to left (Gaskell). groupings were at once established (fig. 151). In another ex- periment with the same reagent, as the application of heat is known to neutralise the action induced by Veratrzn and barium salts, I raised the temperature of the plant chamber, with the result that the beats became regular once more, PLANT RESPONSE On ut LS) (5) Antagonistic actions of actd and alkalt.—But the most remarkable of all the similarities seen in the pulsations of rhythmic tissues of animal and vegetable, is that of the antagonistic actions of acid and alkali. Acid induces in the case of the heart a relaxed or diastolic standstill, whereas Fic. 153. Systolic Arrest of Heart-beat by Dilute NaHO Solution (Gaskell) alkali induces an effect exactly the opposite, the standstill being brought about at systolic contraction (figs. 152 and 153). It is also known that the standstill caused by one of these reagents can be counteracted by the antagonistic action of the other. It is astonishing to find that exactly the same effects are produced by these reagents on the tissue of Desmodium. I first tried the effect of dilute hydro- chloric acid, which, as will be seen, produced an arrest of pulsation in the ‘diastolic’ or relaxed position (fig. 154). I next tried thevetecr of alkali—dilute solution of sodium hydrate—and it will be seen that this produced an arrest of pulsation in the ‘systolic’ or contracted position (fig. 155). In records of the effect of Fic. 154. Arrest of Des- this reagent on other specimens, in reecice weenie which its action had not proceeded tion * of Acid so far, there was a continuous dimi- nution of pulsation with a shifting towards the systole; and when an acid was now applied, the antagonistic character of its action to alkali was clearly shown, by a gradual revival of response, with a_ shifting towards the diastole. In the present record, the systolic standstill caused by alkali was allowed to proceed far, and RHYTHMIC RESPONSE IN PLANT AND ANIMAL 353 yet on application of the acid there is seen to be a slight revival of pulsation and a final arrest towards diastole. Identical nature of rhythmic phenomena in animal and vegetable tissues. —We have thus found the responsive phenomena of cardiac muscle to be in every respect similar to those observed in the rhythmic tissues of Desmodium and Fic. 155. Arrest of Pulsation of Desmodium at ‘ Systole ’ by Application of Dilute Alkali at 7 Acid was next applied at |, and the record shows its antagonistic action. Biophytum, the response of the latter being regarded as practically that of Desmodium in a state of standstill. We have seen that in these rhythmic animal and_ vegetable tissues the fundamental characteristics are identical : (1) Inallof them stimulus gives either maximum response or none. (2) They all exhibit a long refractory period, during which additional stimulus produces apparently no effect. (3) They are all incapable of being tetanised. And further, as regards the influence of various external agencies on the two classes of rhythmic tissues, animal and vegetable, the effects are also remarkably similar. Internal L\” Jak 354 PLANT RESPONSE hydrostatic pressure renews in them rhythmic activity, All of them exhibit under certain circumstances similar cyclic groupings. The effects of chemical reagents are similar on both classes. Rise of temperature quickens the rhythm and reduces the amplitude of pulsation in a manner exactly similar in both ; and, finally, the effects of chemical reagents, even in the matter of antagonistic actions, are alike in the two classes. From a consideration of all these, it would appear that in studying response in rhythmic animal and vegetable tissues, we are dealing, not with two distinct but with a single class of phenomena. We are thus justified in ascribing the rhythmic action of the heart to those same causes which we have found to originate and maintain the rhythm of Desmodium or Biophytum. ‘We have seen that these plants absorb energy continuously from the various forms of stimulus—mechanical, thermal, chemical, and other —to which they are subjected. This absorbed energy remains latent in the tissue, and determines its tonic con- dition, which is simply the sum total of these latent stimu- lating factors. When the sum of these factors exceeds a certain value, it will find expression outwardly in the form of excitatory discharges. This discharge, however, is not single and continuous, but intermittent. After each partial excita- tory discharge, there is a diminution of conductivity and excitability which are only restored gradually. The long refractory period is merely an expression of this peculiar property. Owing to this periodic oscillation of conductivity and excitability, the cczstan¢ latent stimulus finds expression in a rhythmic manner. Under favourable circumstances, there is a large surplus of accumulated energy, and long- continued responses, apparently automatic, are thus produced. Under less favourable conditions, when the stored-up energy is not great, a single stimulus gives rise to a single response, or, when the stimulus is stronger, to a multiple series of rhythmic responses; and this statement is true of tissues exhibiting ‘spontaneous movements, not only in the case of the plant, but also in that of cardiac muscle. RHYTHMIC RESPONSE IN PLANT AND ANIMAL 355 It is impossible to conceive that there could be movement without an exciting cause. Automatism is said to be one of the properties of protoplasm. It will be seen, however, from the evidence which I have adduced in cases where experi- mental investigation is possible, that, strictly speaking, there is no such thing as automatism. Only under the action of a stimulus can a living tissue give responsive indications. The impact of an external stimulus may give rise to an imme- diate expression, or it may partly or wholly be reserved in latent form for subsequent manifestation. ‘Inner stimuli’ are simply external stimuli absorbed previously and held latent. An animal ora plant is thus an accumulator which is constantly storing up energy from external sources,and numerous mani- festations of life—often periodic in their character—are but responsive expressions of energy which has been derived from external sources and held latent in the tissue. SUMMARY The rhythmic tissue of Azophytum may be regarded as equivalent to that of Desmodium in a state of standstill. Both alike, when at standstill, give a single response to a single moderate stimulus, and multiple response to a strong stimulus. Both, when the sum total of latent energy is above par, give apparently ‘automatic’ responses. The rhythmic tissues of both plants exhibit a long re- fractory period. The automatically respending leaflet of Desmodium is incapable of being tetanised. An artificial increase of internal hydrostatic pressure renews pulsation in a Desmodium 7 was previously in a state of standstill. The effect of rise of temperature on Desmodium is to produce a shortening of period and decrease of amplitude of oscillation. The automatic responses of Desmodium often exhibit periodic groupings, of various degrees of complexity. AAZ2 356 PLANT RESPONSE Certain reagents tend to make irregular pulsations in Desmodium regular. Conversely, other reagents, like barium salts, induce irregularity in regular pulsations. The irre- cularity induced by the latter reagent may, however, be counteracted by a rise of temperature. The effects of acids and alkalis on the pulsatory move- ments of Desmodium are antagonistic, acids inducing arrest of pulsation in a relaxed position, while alkalis induce its arrest in a contracted position. The standstill induced by one reagent may therefore be counteracted by the use of the other. By all these, the rhythmic phenomena of the plant are seen to be identical with those of the animal. The pulsation of the animal heart is thus to be ascribed to the sarne causes as bring about and maintain the rhythmic pulsations of Desmodium. BAe TV. ASCENT OF SAP CHAPTER =X XV IIT SUCTIONAL RESPONSE AND ASCENT OF SAP Inadequacy of existing theories of ascent of sap—General considerations regarding cellular activity and resultant propulsion of water—The Shoshungraph— Balanced Shoshungraph for determining variations of suction—Hydrostatic and Hydraulic Methods of Balance. THERE are few phenomena in plant-life which have attracted keener interest and inquiry than that process of transport by which water is carried, from below the surface of the earth to the tops of the tallest trees. The obscurity of the subject is so great, and the secondary co-operating agencies so numerous, that the inquirer is apt to be led into the error of confining his attention to some one of them alone, imagin- ing it to be the principal element in the problem. Jn studying this subject, then, our first effort must be to distin- guish between the essential factor and others which are merely subsidiary. For a statement of the inadequacy of these subsidiary factors to the solution of the problem, it is only necessary to refer to the summary of Strasburger and Pfeffer regarding existing theories of the ascent of sap:! The theory of atmospheric pressure is discredited, inas- much as water is known to be lifted, in certain cases, to many times the height of the water-barometer. The theory of capzllarzty is inadequate, inasmuch as continuous capillaries are absent, and the height to which liquids could be raised by such means would not, moreover, approach that of an ordinary tree. ' Pfeffer, Physiology of Plants, English translation, 1903, vol. i. p. 222, et seg. ; Strasburger, Yext-book of Botany, English translation, 1903, p. 188. 360 PLANT RESPONSE The theory of osmotic action cannot be considered satis- factory, since such action is too slow ; besides which, there is no fixed distribution of osmotic substances, such as would account for the necessary transportation-current. The theory of voot-pressure, again, is open to the objection that it cannot possibly account for the maintenance of a sufficient force during the process of active transpiration, when root-pressure is found to be negative. Moreover, this root- pressure itself requires an explanation. There is, however, another theory, due to Dixon, Joly, and Askenasy, which has apparently more to support it than any of those yet mentioned. According to this, the ascent is brought about by transpiration from the leaves. The fluid in the mesophy] cells of the leaves becomes concentrated by evaporation ; thus osmotic attraction is set up by the leaves, and the suction thereby exerted is supposed to be transmitted backwards, as far as the roots, through cohering columns of water. The difficulties in the way of this theory lie (1) in explaining how a slow osmotic action could produce so rapid a water-current ; (2) in the absence of any conclusive proof that, under actual conditions within the plant, the water- column could have sufficient tensile strength; and, lastly, (3) in the fact which I shall demonstrate, that, when evapo- ration is not taking place in the leaves, the transport of water is still very considerable, and that, besides, other related phenomena, like exudation pressure, continue to take placeeven in the complete absence of evaporative activity in the leaves. I shall, moreover, be able to show that the movement of water often takes place in the plant in a direction opposite to that which would be the case if osmotic action were alone involved. ‘Thus, to quote Pfeffer, ‘a satisfactory explanation of the means by which the transpiration-current is maintained has not yet been brought forward. If no vital actions take part in it, then it is obvious that we have only an incomplete knowledge of the causes at work, and of the relationship of the different factors concerned.’ ! ' Pfeffer, Physiology of Plants, English translation, 1903, p. 224. SUCTIONAL RESPONSE AND ASCENT OF SAP 361 There then remains the question as to whether living cells by some unknown physiological activity might not be instrumental in effecting this transport of water. But the experiments of Hartig, Bohm, and Strasburger have been held to contradict such a possibility. Thus, Strasburger set the cut ends of trees in tubs of poison, such as copper sulphate solution. The poison ascended to the leaves, a distance, in the tallest trees, of twenty-one metres. Now, if such violent protoplasmic poisons ascend the trunk, it is clear that they must kill all the cells lying in their path. That the living cells of the stems could not be necessary to the rise of sap was taken to be a necessary inference from this experiment. Strasburger also killed portions of the stems of living trees by heat, and yet the upper living and leafy portion was found to remain turgid for a few days. Another well-known experiment which was held to negative the theory of protoplasmic activity, was that in which boiling water was poured on the roots, when the plant con- tinued to transpire, in spite of the roots having been killed. From such considerations, Strasburger was led to con- clude that ‘the supposition that the living elements in any way co-operate in the ascent of the transpiration-current is absolutely precluded.’ ! I shall nevertheless show that the ascent of sap is funda- mentally due to the physiological activity of living cells, and that the experiments described above in no way negative this, being, on the contrary, capable of a different, and very satisfactory, explanation. Many difficulties connected with the problem of the ascent of sap will be found to disappear, when the physiological activity of living tissues is once clearly established as the essential factor. But a vague assumption of protoplasmic activity will not be sufficient for the elucidation of the phenomenon. It will be necessary to show further how this excitatory activity is initiated, and by what means a definite-directioned flow is imparted to the sap. ' Strasburger, Zext-dook of Botany, English translation, 1903, p. 188. 362 PLANT RESPONSE We have seen in Chapter XXI. that when a strong stimulus is applied to the base of any organ, say a stem, a multiple series of excitatory waves is propagated onwards, such multiple responses being detected by electrotactile or electromotive pulsations. It was also shown in Chapter XXVI. that such multiple, passing into automatic, response, may be induced by the action of a constant stimulus. It was further demonstrated in Chapter XXI. that these excita- tory waves, and the concomitant cell-to-cell contraction, would produce a movement of water forwards, along the ‘direction of propagation. This series of excitatory waves, proceeding from the base of the organ, and propelling water forwards, must then cause a deficit of water behind. If, however, the base of the organ be kept supplied with water, this deficit will be made up by suction. It is thus seen that by such rhythmic activity a one- directioned movement of water may be produced. Just as the various effects produced by multiple or autonomous response—in, for example, the electromotive and electro- tactile responses, and in the multiple mechanical responses of Desmodium—give us an indication of the degree of rhythmic activity exhibited by the tissue, so, in the rate of this water- movement also, we have an additional means of measure- ment. This would be analogous to the measurement of the rhythmic activity of the heart by a determination of the rate of flow of the circulating blood. In the case of the plant this rate of movement may be measured, either by means of the propulsion of water forwards or by the suction exerted behind. The ascent of sap in the plant, then, may be brought about by the rhythmic activity of the tissue. How this activity is initiated will be discussed later. Meanwhile it is clear that if the movement of sap be really an expression of protoplasmic activity, then any physiological modifica- tion which tends to increase that activity will also tend to increase the rate of movement; and pari passu any physio- logical condition which tends to depress the activity, will SUCTIONAL RESPONSE AND ASCENT OF SAP 363 correspondingly express itself in a diminished rate of move- ment. The movement of sap is thus taken to be another expression of that autonomous activity (multiple response) of the plant, which we have already seen exhibited locally by the motile tissue of Desmodium. The conclusive test of this would lie in proving that all those agencies which acted in a given way on the autonomous response of Desmodium, act also in the same way in modifying the rate of movement of sap. In other words, just as an exciting reagent will in the one case induce a greater amplitude, frequency, or both, of oscillation, above the normal, so in the other the excitatory nature of a given reagent may be expected to exhibit itself by an increase above the normal, in the rate of flow. A depressing reagent, on the contrary, should produce the opposite effect in both. That is to say, just as we may study the multiple or rhythmic excitability of a tissue through mechanical, electromotive, or electrotactile response, so here, in hydraulic response, or the determination of changes in the rate of flow of sap, we have an independent mode of investigating the same phenomenon. The great difficulty of this investigation lies in the absence of a method by which these changes can be imme- diately recorded. In other words, we require some simple means of making a direct record, which will show, in a continuous manner, the changes produced by the various agencies, enabling us to distinguish their immediate effects, after effects, time-relations, and so on. We have seen that the propulsion of water forward by the tissue is attended by a suction behind.’ The quantity of water sucked up in a definite period will therefore give us an indication of the rate of movement of sap in the plant. Thus from the readings afforded by the water-index of a potometer, and the times at which such readings are taken, we may derive ! The forward movement of water, and the suction exerted, in the same tissue, are not necessarily equal in all cases. Part of the water sucked up may be deviated to increase the turgidity of the cells themselves. Suction may neverthe- less be taken as a measure, other things being equal, of rhythmic activity. 364 ‘PLANT RESPONSE the rate of movement. But these readings are necessarily discontinuous, and important phases of change are thus apt to be overlooked. They are, again, subject to error; nor do they give us, at sight, the rate of flow, nor the changes in that rate,at any given moment. In order todetermine such varia- tions, a laborious process of construction of curves, from the experimental data, must, generally speaking, be undertaken. It was therefore necessary to devise an apparatus by means of which curves might be obtained direct, so that a mere inspection would be sufficient to inform us as to the normal rate of suction, and the influence of external factors on that rate. This suction will be shown to be a physio- logical phenomenon ; its variation, therefore, will enable us to measure the physiological influence of various external agencies. It must be borne in mind that under ordinary conditions there is a normal rate of suction. The incidence of an external stimulus will change this rate, and the varzatzon of rate which results is thus a measure of the effect produced by the stimulus. Similarly, we measure a force by noting the variation which it produces in the rate of movement of a uniformly moving body. The variation of the rate of suction may thus be taken as constituting a form of response to stimulus, which for the sake of simplicity we shall designate as Suctional Response. The Shoshungraph.— For the purpose of subjecting the plant to various conditions, and also in order to obtain the record of the resultant suctional response, I have constructed an apparatus to which I have given the name of the Shoshungraph.' \t consists of (1) an arrangement by which the specimen may be rapidly subjected to the action of different excitatory or depressing agents; (2) a potometric tube, by which the constant changes of suctional activity are measured ; (3) a contrivance by means of which the move- ments of the water-index, with their time-relations, are recorded. The principal parts of this instrument are shown diagrammatically in fig. 156. V is the plant-vessel, in which ' From Sanskrit, S#oshat = suction, SUCTIONAL RESPONSE AND ASCENT OF SAP 365 the specimen is mounted by means of a water-tight india- rubber cork. The vessel is closed at the bottom also by means of a large cork through which enter four tubes. One of these, controlled by the stop-cock A, is connected with the capillary potometer-tube. The second, controlled by 4’, leads to the compensating vessel C, filled with water. The interior ends of these two tubes reach almost to the top of the plant-vessel. The third, with stop-cock B’, leads to Fic. 156. Diagrammatic Representation of the Shoshungraph v, plant-vessel ; C, compensator; R, reservoir. The potometer-tube is controlled by stop-cock, A ; compensator by a’; and reservoir by B’. B is the stop-cock of the outflow pipe; D, recording drum driven by clockwork ; P, collar carrying recording pen, seen magnified above. The water-index is followed by appropriate manipulation of the wheel, w. the reservoir R, from which water at various temperatures or different chemical reagents may be introduced. The fourth is an outlet-tube controlled by the stop-cock B. Adjustment for Unbalanced Record. — The great difficulty in connection with delicate experiments arises from the presence of air-bubbles in the plant-vessel, which are not easy to expel. This is done, however, by means of an escape- tube, with a stop-cock, which runs through the upper cork, 366 PLANT RESPONSE and is not shown inthe figure. The stop-cock A’ is opened, A, B, and B’ being closed. Water from the compensator C thus passes into the plant-vessel, and the air-bubbles which have been accumulating at the top of the vessel escape, with the water which is driven out at the escape-tube. When all are. expelled then the stop-cock of the escape-tube, and A’, are closed. The potometer stop-cock A is now opened, and the water-index is adjusted at any point desired, by manipulation of the stop-cocks B and A’. A temporary opening of the stop-cock A’ of the compensator C causes the water-index to move to the left ; whereas, when the stop-cock B, of the outflow pipe, is opened, it moves to the right. After this preliminary adjustment the stop-cocks A’, B, and B’ are closed, the potometer tap A being kept open ; the movement of the water-index per unit-time now gives us the normal rate of suction of the specimen. We come next to the question of making a direct record of the rate of movement. For this purpose, a writing pen is fitted on the potometric tube, by means of a brass collar. This brass collar has a rectangular opening, which enables us to watch the water-index. It has also stretched across it a fine wire, which is kept always coincident with the water- index. This wire is parallel with, and placed vertically above, the recording pen. The collar is attached to a thread which passes round small pulleys. One end of this thread carries a counterpoise and the other is wound round a wheel, w, which can be so manipulated as to make the index-wire follow the movements of the water-column. When the wheel is wound, the index moves to the right ; when it is slightly released, the weight of the counterpoise makes it move to the left. The weighted recording pen rests with its point on a revolving drum, D, covered with paper for the record ; this drum is kept revolving by clockwork at a known and adjustable speed. When the water-index is followed in the way described, there is produced a direct record of water- movement in the plant. A curve is thus traced, the ordinate of which represents the quantity of water sucked up, and the SUCTIONAL RESPONSE AND ASCENT OF SAP 367 abscissa the time. The slope of the curve thus gives the rate of movement. As long as the suction is uniform, the slope remains constant. If any exciting agency increases the rate of suction, there is an immediate flexure in the curve, which thus becomes steeper. A depressing agent lessens the slope of the curve. And when suction is abolished, the record becomes horizontal. By this arrangement, then, we are enabled, simply and accurately, to obtain a direct and continuous record ; and as the necessity for taking readings is obviated, a large number of experiments can be performed very quickly, with little trouble. The flexure in the curve affords immediate visible indication of the effect of any particular agency. The value of each division of the ordinate is found once for all by determining the volume of unit-length of the potometer-tube. A previous determination at the beginning of the experiment of the rate of movement of the drum, gives us the time- value of each division of the abscissa. Knowing these, we can determine the absolute rate of suction at any period of the curve required. Responsive variations of suction are more easily detected when the normal curve is almost equally inclined to the ordinate and abscissa—that is to say, when it makes an angle of about 45° with either. This is most easily accomplished if we keep the potometer-tube always the same, and merely adjust the speed of the drum. The Balanced Shoshungraph.— According to the simple method of making records which has just been described, we observe the responsive effect by means of flexures produced in the curve under the action of various agencies. If the effect of an agency be slight, the change in the slope of the curve will be proportionately small and liable to escape detection. In order to bring the sensitiveness of the instru- ment to its highest, I have devised the Method of Balance, by which the slightest responsive variation is made to exhibit itself in a marked manner. For the purpose of many delicate investigations, it is not so necessary to know the normal rate of suction, as the varatzons positive and negative in that rate. 368 PLANT RESPONSE An agency which induces the positive variation will then be excitatory, while that which induces the negative is depressing. In order to carry out my investigations along these lines, I have employed two different methods of balance. In one—the Hydrostatic Method of Balance—the natural suction of the plant is arrested by a counter-hydrostatic pressure suitably applied. The effect of an external agent is now studied by the direction, positive or negative, and the extent to which it disturbs the static equilibrium thus established. Experiments carried out on this method will be described in the next chapter. The second—or Hydraulic Method of Balance—depends upon an application of compensation, by means of which under normal conditions the water-index is kept stationary, though the suctional movement in the plant is in no way disturbed. According to this hydraulic method, the normal rate of suction, when balanced, gives rise to a neutral, or horizontal, line in the record; while an exciting agent produces an inclination upwards ; and a depressing agent a declination downwards. The balance, by which under normal conditions the neutral line is secured, is obtained by allowing water to enter the plant-vessel from the compensator C at a rate exactly equal to that of its withdrawal by suction. In practice, this adjustment is roughly made by opening the stop-cock A’ in connection with C, to a greater or less extent. Over-balance causes movement of the water-index to the left ; under-balance to the right ; and when the adjust- ment is perfect, the water-index becomes stationary. After making the preliminary adjustment, the final balance may be obtained, by very careful and gradual movement of the com- pensating reservoir up or down. When the reservoir is raised the flow is,increased, owing to the greater difference of level established, as between the reservoir and the plant- vessel. The stand on which the vessel C is placed is pro- vided with a rack and pinion, by means of which the necessary adjustment of height is made. Introduction of changed conditions.—We have thus SUCTIONAL RESPONSE AND ASCENT OF SAP — 369 seen how the normal rate of suction and its variations may be recorded accurately. The next difficulty to be overcome is that of introducing the changed conditions without creating any disturbance, thus practically maintaining the continuity of record. It will be necessary to observe, among other things, the immediate and after-effects of cold and heat, as well as those of various chemical reagents. This is accom- plished by the right manipulation of the four stop-cocks. Let it be supposed that we wish to study the immediate and after-effects of cold. Up to this time the stop-cocks A and A’ have been opened—B and B’ being closed—and the balanced horizontal record taken. For the sake of simplicity I refer to the stop-cock A’ as the only one which opens and closes the communication with the compensator C. In reality, however, there is a second stop-cock in its neighbourhood, by which the balancing adjustment is first made, A’ being employed for opening or closing communication under such an adjustment. The reservoir R is now filled with cold water ; Aand A’ are next closed—thus arresting the water- index—and B and B’ opened. The water then leaves the plant-vessel by the overflow pipe, and its place is taken by cold water from R. After this, the stop-cocks B and B’ axe once more closed, and A and A’ opened. The index, being now released, indicates by its movement the excitatory ' ‘or depressing effect of cold. It must be remembered that the index was previously adjusted to balance. Should the effect of cold prove to be excitatory, the rate of suction would be increased ; under-balanced by the supply of water ‘from C, the index would move to the right, thus giving positive response. If, on the contrary, the effect should be depressing, the rate of suction would be decreased, and the water from the compensator would produce an over-balance, causing a movement of the index to the left, or negative response. By now filling the reservoir R with water at the ordinary temperature, and repeating the operation, the original condition is re-established, and the effect of re-esta- blishment of old conditions observed. In a similar manner, BB 370 PLANT RESPONSE we may study the immediate and after-effects of rise of temperature, and of the various chemical reagents. Fic. 157. Photograph of Shoshungraph \, plant-vessel ; R, reservoir ; C, compensator, whose balancing height is adjusted by rack and pinion, s; kK, key for manipulation of four-way stop-cock ; Pp, recording pen, [with counterpoise M, manipulated by wheel, w. The drum is rotated by the clock at uniform speed. SUCTIONAL RESPONSE AND ASCENT OF SAP Val In order to make the explanation easier, I have described all these stop-cocks and their serial opening and closing separately ; but this arrangement, apart from its clumsiness, would involve a certain loss of time. In many of these experiments, it must be remembered, it is necessary to know the immediate effect produced. I have therefore simplified the procedure by the use of a special key, K, by turning which, in one direction or another, the requisite alternate openings and closings are accomplished. Thus, on turning the key-handle to the left, A and A’ are opened, and B and B’ closed. The balanced record is now taken. As the handle is now being turned to the right, the stop-cocks A and A’ are first closed, arresting the index. Con- tinued turning to the right opens B and B’, by which the modi- fying reagent is introduced into the plant-vessel. A sudden turning of the key to the left now closes B and B’, and opens A and A’, thus releasing the index and enabling the record to be taken once more, under the changed conditions. The whole process is thus made so rapid, that modified conditions can be established, and the record renewed, within the short interval of less than one minute. The photograph of the completed apparatus is seen in fig. 157. Having now given in detail all the experimental arrangements, I shall in the next chapter describe the physiological modifications induced by different agencies, as exhibited by the suctional response. The periodic variation of ascent of sap may be recorded continuously and automatically by photography. Another method of recording transpiration will also be found described on page 472. SUMMARY This chapter gives a description of the Shoshungraph, by means of which the rate of suction and its variations may be indicated and recorded, The sensitiveness of the apparatus is very much increased by the Hydrostatic and Hydraulic Methods of Balance. eee CHAPTER XXEX MODIFICATION OF SUCTIONAL RESPONSE Effect of temperature on suction by three methods of inquiry: (1) Unbalanced method of Shoshungraph: (a) Action of cold—(é) Action of moderate rise of temperature—(2) Method of Hydrostatic Balance: (a) Action of cold—Re- versal of normal direction of flow—(é) Action of warm water—(3) Method of Hydraulic Balance : (a) Action of cold—(é) Effect of warm water—Explana- tion of suction when the root is killed by boiling water —Stimulation renews suctional activity in plant whose suction has come to a standstill—Osmotic versus excitatory action—Abolition of suction by poison—Suctional activity continued until whole plant is killed by poison. I SHALL now proceed to prove that the movement of water in plants is mainly due to rhythmic excitation of the tissue, and that evaporation from the leaves, osmotic action, and so on, are only co-operating factors, of subsidiary importance. We have seen in Chapter XXI. that any part of a stem when excited will.become the seat of rhythmic activity, and that this pulsatory excitation causes movement of water. I shall now demonstrate a similar phenomenon by means of suc- tional response, eliminating from some of the typical ex- periments all auxiliary factors, such as osmotic action and evaporation from leaves. I have shown in the last chapter how the effect of suctional activity may be continuously recorded by means of the Shoshungraph. We saw also that the effects of various agents, exciting or depressing, were to be detected, under the unbalanced method, by appropriate variation in the slope of the curve. By the balanced method, whether hydrostatic or hydraulic, the derangement of the balance upwards indicates an increase of activity, and its derangement downwards a decrease. I shall first demon- strate the fact that these observations, though obtained by such various methods, are all reliable and mutually consis- MODIFICATION OF SUCTIONAL RESPONSE 373 tent, by subjecting the plant to the action of an agent whose general effect is well known, and recording the results by all three methods. For this purpose we shall take the influence of low and moderately high temperatures. Modification of suctional response by various agencies, We have seen that any sudden variation of temperature acts as a stimulus in itself. Thus, if we touch the pulvinus of Mimosa or Biophytum with ice, there is a responsive twitch. This may be taken as the preliminary effect. Pro- longed application of cold, however, abolishes excitability. If the ascent of sap be really a phenomenon of excitation, we may expect to find a sudden application of cold, appro- priately made, producing a preliminary augmentation, followed by the depression and arrest of suction. An application of hot water might be expected, on the other hand, to bring about the contrary effect, that is to say, an increase in the rate of suction. I shall now describe the experimental results obtained by the three methods. Effect of temperature on suction: (1) Unbalanced Method. (a) Action of cold—As | did not know what might be the effect of injury on the suctional activity of the plant, I selected intact specimens for my first experiments. For this purpose I took cuttings of Cvoton,a plant whose stem when placed in water will develop roots in a few weeks’ time. When the roots were well developed, the specimen was fitted in its place in the apparatus. In other cases, I took pot-grown specimens and placed them in water, so that the earth was dissolved away. Violence to the rootlets was thus avoided.. I may here state, however, that I found, in the course of these experiments, that there is no essential difference between the effects exhibited in such intact plants and those observed in the case of cut branches. All that is necessary in the latter case is that the specimen should be mounted in the apparatus and left for some time, in order that the effect of the disturbance caused by cut may pass. The record afforded by a specimen thus mounted gives the normal rate of suction. The attainment of constancy of 374 PLANT RESPONSE external conditions is gauged by the uniform inclination to the curve, and it may be well to mention here that through- out the investigation every experiment was begun with this test. The normal rate of suction, in the first experiment, in a Croton at the temperature of the room (23° C.), was eight cubic mm. per minute. On now applying cold water at a temperature of 4° C. to the root, by appropriate manipulation of the stop-cock, the rate showed the preliminary excitatory effect, due to sudden variation of temperature, by an increased Fic. 158. Effect of Cold on Suction The first part of the record shows the normal rate of suction at 23° C, Asterisk marks moment of application of cold water at 4° C., which is seen to produce a preliminary exaltation, followed by arrest of suction. Abscissa represents time in minutes; ordinate, the quantity of water sucked up in milligrammes or cubic mm. rate during the first two minutes, of eighteen cubic mm. per minute, or 2°25 times the normal value. But this temporary exaltation gradually passed away, till there was an almost complete arrest, ten minutes after the first application (fig. 158). This arrest by cold was not found to be permanent ; for it disappeared on the return to a higher temperature, as will be seen in the first part of the next figure (fig. 159), which was taken after water at 23° C. had been substituted for the cold water in the vessel. In this second curve, the rate of suction is found to return almost to the normal MODIFICATION OF SUCTIONAL RESPONSE 375 degree, being now about seven instead of eight cubic mm. per minute. (6) Action of moderate rise of temperature—I1 next tried the effect of a rise of temperature. This experiment was per- formed with the same specimen as the last, in which the return normal rate of suction at 23° C. had already been determined to be seven cubic mm. per minute. On now Fic. 159. Curve showing Normal Suction at 23° C.,_ Increased Suction at 35° C., and the After-effect Fic. 160. Method of Hydrostatic persisting on Return to Balance Normal Temperature A short length of stem, P, has its as- This experiment was carried censional water-movement balanced out on the same specimen by superincumbent water-column of as the last. variable height. applying water at 35° C. it will be seen that a very steep rise was induced in the curve, indicating an increased suctional rate of fifty-eight cubic mm. per minute, more than eight times the rate at 23° C. On now once more substituting water at 23° C. the rate became lowered, though not to the original degree (fig. 159). It must be remembered, with regard to this, that the movement of sap depends on the cell- activity of the entire plant, and that the tissue has by this 376 PLANT RESPONSE time absorbed some quantity of hot water, which cannot immediately be displaced by the water of ordinary tempera- ture which is applied at the roots. The rate of suction, therefore, could not at once revert to the normal, but must be expected for some short period to show a slight enhancement. The curve shows that on the return to 23° C. the rate fell from fifty-eight to fourteen instead of to the original eight cubic mm. per minute. (2) Method of Hydrostatic Balance: (@) Action of cold.—For this experiment I took a Cyvofon stem cut at both ends, the lower end being placed in the plant-vessel of the apparatus. There was now found to be a _ considerable movement of water upwards. This movement was arrested by suitable hydrostatic pressure, the upper end of the stem being connected with an india-rubber tubing filled with water and ending in a funnel (fig. 160). To prevent evapora- tion, the surface of the water in the funnel was covered with a thin film of oil. Rather a high hydrostatic pressure was required to produce a balance. When the pressure of a column of 75 cm. was applied, there was still a movement of water upwards in the tissue, at so great a rate as ten cubic mm. per minute. In this experiment, then, the root having been cut off, there is still a considerable propulsion of water upwards through the stem. It is thus clear that root-pressure is not the essential factor in the ascent of sap. As the leaves had also been cut off, and the cut end of the stem covered by water sealed with a film of oil, evaporation from the leaves, and the osmotic action thereby produced, are also seen to be excluded. These, like root-pressure, therefore, cannot consti- tute the essential factors in the process of suction. But since, on the contrary, any small length of the stem is competent to show this water-movement, the required activity must reside in the tissue of the stem. A balance was finally obtained, by the pressure of a water-column of 105 cm. This equilibrium is not to be re- garded as merely the result of equality between the upward MODIFICATION OF SUCTIONAL RESPONSE 377 pressure, exerted by suction from below, and the downward pressure of the superincumbent water-column. There are reasons for thinking that there may also be an important additional factor, of opposed rhythmic activities, balancing each other. For when a cut branch is placed in water, the lower end becomes over-turgid, and this, as we know, is a condition for the initiation of rhythmic activity. The upper end of the branch not being so turgid, activity will - be greater below than above. We therefore obtain a movement of water from the more to the less active. But if the leafy end of the branch be immersed in water, and the cut end held in the air, it is known that the direction of the flow becomes reversed. This is evidently due to the fact that it is now the upper end which is over-turgid, and there- fore relatively the more active. Similarly, in the case of the balance described, the activity of the lower end, which deter- mines the flow upwards, was opposed and balanced by the increased activity of the upper half, induced by increased hydrostatic pressure.' The experiment which I am about to describe, besides demonstrating the effects of cold and warmth, also lends strong support to the view that the direc- tion of the resultant movement of sap is determined by the relative activities of the two ends of the stem. When the balance had been obtained, as already described, with a pressure of water of 105 cm., the record on the drum became horizontal, as has been explained. Cold water was now applied to the specimen at its lower end. As sudden cooling constitutes a stimulus, while its continued action produces depression, we should expect a transient augmentation of the activity of the lower end of the specimen, followed by its diminution and arrest. The record should therefore show a preliminary movement of water upwards, followed by the reversal of the current, which should now, as the permanent effect of cold applied below, be from the ' Rhythmic activity is, in general, increased by an increase in the hydrostatic pressure. But there is a limit to this. Excessive pressure, above a certain critical point, is found to depress rhythmic activity (p. 350). 378 PLANT RESPONSE more active upper, to the less active lower end, or downwards. The record will be seen to verify this anticipation in every particular (fig. 161). Cold water was applied below—repre- sented by x in the record—which before application was horizontal. After this we observe a movement of water upwards, at arate of four cubic mm. per minute. The upward flow continues for about seven minutes, by which time the Fic. 161. Record obtained by Method of Hydrostatic Balance of Successive Applications of Cold and Warm Water After obtaining static balance, with horizontal record not shown in the figure, cold water was applied below at moment x. The transitory excitation thus occasioned causes movement of water upwards, followed by depression and reversal of flow downwards. Warm water next applied at ¢ causes second reversal and flow of water upwards. activity of the lower end of the specimen has become so depressed as to cause reversal of the flow, which is now from above downwards at an average rate of about six cubic mm. per minute. This record was taken continuously for some time, when hot water was quickly substituted for cold, in the plant-vessel. A cross marks this point in the record. ° (6) Action of warm water.—The result was not only that MODIFICATION OF SUCTIONAL RESPONSE 379 its sluggishness was now obviated, but that the lower end of the specimen was actually rendered the more excitable of the two, and we observe a second reversal of the direction of current, which now flows upwards. (3) Method of Hydraulic Balance.—This Hydraulic Method of Balance is much easier to carry out than the arrest of movement by hydrostatic pressure. It does not, moreover, in any way interfere with the normal movement of water through the plant. For the balance is obtained and the index rendered stationary, by the simple device already explained, of allowing a subsidiary flow of water to enter the plant-vessel, at a rate exactly sufficient to compensate for the loss by ascent of sap. (a) Action of cold—This experiment was performed on a leafy branch of Croton. The balanced horizontal record was first taken at 22° C., after which ice-cold water was passed into the vessel. A record was made of the immediate or preliminary excitatory effect of this cold water on the rate of suction, and continued during the return of the water to the temperature of the room. In this record, then, we shall find, besides the immediate, the continued effect of cold, and subsequently the effect of gradual restoration to an ordinary temperature. In the present record (fig. 162) we see the transient and permanent effects of cold exhibited as before. According to the method of Hydraulic Balance, as has been explained, an ascending line in the record means a positive variation, or increase of the rate of suction over the normal. A descending line, on the contrary, denotes a negative variation, or diminution of the rate of suction below the normal. And a horizontal line shows return to the original rate. In the first, or upper, of the two curves in fig. 162, the immediate effect of cold is seen in a very marked positive variation. After the lapse of about five minutes, the effect of continued cold is seen in the depression, which shows itself by the reversal of the curve. This depression continues, till the temperature of the vessel has returned to that of the room, which takes place in the course of about forty minutes. 380 PLANT RESPONSE The normal rate of suction is now re-established, as seen in the fact that the record becomes horizontal. (6) Effect of warm water.—I next tried the effect on the same specimen of water at a higher temperature, falling by degrees to the temperature of the room. We should expect in such a case, to obtain the excitatory effect of rise of temperature, with subsequent approach to the normal, with- out any reversal, indicative of the transition from exaltation to depression, such as was observed in the case of application of cold. From the second and lower of the two curves FIG. 162. Record, obtained by Method of Hydraulic Balance, of Successive Effects of Cold and Warm Water shown in fig. 162, it will be seen that this is the case. The curve at first rises abruptly, and it continues to rise, though with decreased speed, till, when the temperature of the vessel is once more normal, it becomes horizontal, indicating the resumption of the original rate of suction. We have thus seen, by three different methods of inquiry those namely of the Unbalanced Shoshungraph, the Hydro- static Balance, and the Hydraulic Balance—that the re- cords, though of different forms, exhibit effects of which the interpretations are identical. For delicate experiments, the MODIFICATION OF SUCTIONAL RESPONSE 381 Hydraulic Method of Balance will be found most sensitive : for ordinary purposes, however, the unbalanced Shoshun- graph is simple and efficient ; and in the investigations which follow I shall use this method only. Explanation of suction, when the root is killed by boiling water.—I shall now take up the apparently anoma- lous case in which, when the root has been killed, by pouring boiling water over it, the suction of the plant is nevertheless maintained. In such an experiment, the normal record was first taken, and on allowing boiling water to enter the plant- chamber there was a steep rise in the record, showing the excitatory action due to the application of hot water. The boiling water was now passed in continuously for several, minutes, so as to ensure the killing of that portion of the plant which was immersed in the vessel. On allowing the water in the vessel to return to the temperature of the room, it was found that suction continued, at an even greater than the original normal rate. This result would at first appear to show that protoplas- mic activity had nothing to-do with the ascent of sap. And the objection would have been fatal, if the rhythmic activity which produces suction had been confined to the roots alone. But such activity is present to a greater or less extent throughout every zone of the plant, and it is by the com- bined action of all these that the ascensional movement is maintained (p. 376). Thus, when hot water is poured on the root, its first effect is a sudden increase of the activity of that organ, by which warm water is carried to the higher zones, there as a stimulating agency to increase this rhythmic activity. It must be remembered that on reaching the stem above the vessel, the hot water itself is considerably cooled. Hence the only portion of the plant which is killed is that- which is actually immersed in the boiling water, or in immediate contiguity with it. The unkilled portions above continue their suctional activity unabated. I have said that the suction, on the return of the water to its old temperature, continued to take place at a greater 382 PLANT RESPONSE than the original rate. This was due to the fact that, instead of the extremely attenuated channels of the root-hairs, through which suction normally takes place, there was now substituted the whole mass of the root, acting virtually as a wet rag, tied round the base of the living stem; and indeed it was found that, whereas the stem outside the vessel was turgid, the portion within was limp and soft. The mass of water which it was thus possible to suck up directly, by means of the broad-sectioned stem, was evidently much greater than could have been the case through the interven- tion of the resistant organically conducting channels of the rootlets. We must not forget the obvious fact that a plant is a colony of more or less independent living cells, each of which maintains its physiological activity as an individual. The death of one group does not necessarily, therefore, arrest the physiological activity of its neighbours. The plant is finally killed only when every one of its cellular elements has undergone death. Further proof that suction is an excitatory response.— We have seen in the case of rhythmic Desmodzum, when it is kept for a long time under unfavourable circumstances, that its activity comes to a stop owing to the run-down of stored-up energy. We also saw how the application of thermal stimulus would re-initiate this activity. Again,if we keep a cut branch of any plant in water, after a few days its suctional activity, as is well known, disappears. This abolition of suction is attributed to the blocking of the cut end by mucilage and bacterial growths, and the making of a fresh section is found to renew the activity. But, though the blocking of the cut end of the stem by outgrowths does, no doubt, obstruct the passage of water, yet the total abolition of suction may not be due to this cause alone. It may be induced, in part at least, by depres- sion of the rhythmic activity of the tissue, owing to the run- down of its latent energy. The making of a fresh section does not decide this question, for, in doing this, we apply MODIFICATION OF SUCTIONAL RESPONSE 38 ios) the strong mechanical stimulus of a cut. I therefore devised an experiment which appears to show that this run-down of energy does in such a case constitute a factor in the abolition of suction. Without in any way disturbing the mucilaginous end of the stem, which had ceased to exhibit its suctional activity, I supplied it with water somewhat above the ordinary temperature. This thermal stimulation at once initiated renewed suctionai activity with great vigour, just as its rhythmic mechanical activity was renewed by Desmodium on the application of similar stimulus. Osmotic versus excitatory action.—Though, under the co-operation of a favourable disposition of osmotic substances, the suctional activity of the tissue may be increased, yet I have shown that suction is normally maintained even with- out the co-operation of this factor (p. 376). I shall now proceed to show that this suction may increase even in opposition to osmotic action. And such a demonstration will further prove the excitatory physiological nature of the processes which bring about the ascent of sap. Among various solutions of salt, some are physiologically neutral in their effects ;! of these, potassium nitrate may be taken as an example. Others, again, like strong solutions of sodium chloride, act as excitatory agents. The application of this last reagent is known to initiate rhythmic excitation in animal tissues. Similar effects have been shown to be brought about by this reagent, in the case of Lzophytum and Desmodium. Thus, in a strong solution of potassium nitrate, we have a reagent whose physiological action is more or less neutral while its osmotic action is pronounced, and in a strong solution of common salt we have an agent which is both excitatory and osmotic at the same time. If, then, we apply KNO, solution to the base of a cut stem, placed in the Shoshungraph, water will be osmotically withdrawn from the plant, in opposition t® normal suction, and the ' It should, however, be remembered that solutions, even of inactive salts, above a certain strength, will induce physiological depression, and thus bring about diminution of transpiration. 384 PLANT RESPONSE normal suctional rate will be somewhat reduced. This is seen in the accompanying record (fig. 163), which I obtained with a cut branch of Croton. The normal rate of suction was in this case twenty-six cubic mm. per minute. After the application of potassium nitrate solution this was found to be reduced to seventeen cubic mm. per minute. But if, instead of this, we apply strong solution of sodium chloride, two antagonistic effects will be produced. One, due to Fic. 163. Effect of Strong Fic. 164. Effect of Strong NaCl KNO, Solution Solution The first record shows the nor- The first record shows the nor- mal and the second the de- mal and the second the ex- pressed rate of suction caused alted rate of suction caused by the reagent. by the reagent. osmotic action, will oppose suction; and the other, due to the excitatory nature of the reagent, will accelerate suction. The resultant effect will, then, be modified by the excitability of the experimental plant itself. In some cases we should expect to find the excitatory reaction predomi- nant, and in others the osmotic. In repeating the experi- ment, on different specimens of Croton, I have found these theoretical inferences to be fully verified. As the more interesting of the two cases, I give a record (fig. 164) in MODIFICATION OF SUCTIONAL RESPONSE 385 which the excitatory effect is shown by the very great increase in the rate of suction induced after the application of the reagent. We have here a very great enhancement of the ascensional movement of water in spite of the osmotic attraction of the solution, which alone would have retarded the normal rate. The action of poisonous reagents.—In studying the effect of poison on the rhythmic activity of Desmodium, we found that this was modified by the tonic condition of the plant. Thus a vigorous specimen was shown to be much less affected by poison than one which was weakly. Certain poisons, again, act more quickly than others in inducing the death of the plant. We have also seen, in that experi- ment in which the root was killed with hot water, that upper and unkilled portions of a specimen will continue to exhibit suctional activity when lower parts are killed ; and also that, in general, the killed area offers no barrier to the passage of water through its dead tissues. That a poison can easily pass through killed tissues, owing to the suctional activity of cells higher up, we have seen in our experiments on Desmodium, when the cut end of the petiole was placed in copper sulphate solution (p. 326). It is fortunate that in this case, during the ascent of poison, we have areas, the activity of which is indicated visibly by the rhythmic motile indications of the pulvini of the inserted lateral leaflets. That copper sulphate solution arrests rhythmic activity and induces death, is seen by the rapid stoppage of pulsation, when we apply it directly on the pulvini of the pulsating leaflets. When it is applied, however, at the cut end of the petiole, the arrest of pulsation takes place much later, this delay being due to the time taken for the poison to ascend through the intervening distance. This shows clearly that successive zones are killed one after another, and that the death of a point below does not stop the suction above. From this experiment it is evident that the application of poison at the root, or the cut end of a stem, does not in general arrest cc 386 PLANT RESPONSE suction, until the whole plant is killed. And from the Shoshungraphic records we find that the final arrest occurs after an appropriately long period. In this connection, I shall describe some very interesting results of rapid arrest of suction, which I have often obtained by the action of poison. I was already familiar with a fact which I had come across while studying the effects of various chemical reagents on the longitudinal response of radial organs namely, that death was attended in such cases either by an abnormal contraction or by an abnormal relaxation. These two effects were liable, again, to be modified by the Fic. 165. Effect of Copper Sulphate Solution The first part of the record shows the normal rate o1 suction. ‘The asterisk denotes the time of application of the poisonous reagent. tonic condition of the tissue. In now studying the effect of solution of copper sulphate on the suctional activity of Croton, | noticed certain peculiarities in the record, which appeared to be related to the results just described. These peculiarities, it should be mentioned, were specially notice- able in those specimens which were experimented on during the month of April, that is to say, at the end of the Indian spring. In a particular experiment the normal suctional rate had been fifteen cubic mm. per minute. On the application of copper sulphate, the suctional movement was quickly MODIFICATION OF SUCTIONAL RESPONSE 387 arrested, and this was followed almost immediately by a slight movement-in the negative direction, showing that, by some spasmodic contraction, water was being expelled from the tissue. This phase was succeeded by an almost com- plete arrest of suction, there being now only the feeblest ascensional movement (fig. 165). Within a short period after this, on washing off the poisonous reagent, it was found that the arrest had been temporary only, suction being renewed at the rate of eleven, instead of the normal fifteen, cubic mm. per minute. I applied the poison once more, and allowed it to act for thirty-six hours. The arrest was then found to be permanent —that is to say, the substitution of fresh water induced no revival of response, the plant being killed throughout. Strasburger, as we have seen, in his experiments on the effect of poisonous reagents on plants, found that the reagent is carried to the top of the tallest tree; from this fact it was inferred that since all the cells in the path of the poisonous solution must necessarily be killed by its action, therefore the activity of living cells was not the essential factor in the ascent of sap. But I have proved that the ascent of sap is brought about, not by any localised group of cells in a particular region, but by cells which extend throughout the length of the plant. Even after some of these have died, therefore, by the access of poison, those above are still active, and will continue to exhibit suction till they in their turn are finally killed. It will thus be evident that the movement of ascent cannot be completely abolished till the poison has effectively reached the very top. As all the living cells are actively concerned in the work of suction, this con- veyance of poison to the top of the plant is what was to be expected. Only after such conveyance, indeed, could permanent arrest possibly take place, and, in fact, Strasburger himself mentions that the movement of water did come to a stop when the poison reached the top of the tree. cEe2 388 PLANT RESPONSE SUMMARY Rhythmic activity being as a rule exalted by rise of temperature, suctional response, as one of its effects, also undergoes an increase. Suction being an expression of excitatory response, the direction of the resultant movement of sap is determined by the relative excitabilities of the two ends of a tissue. Under certain circumstances, the normal direction of movement of sap may be reversed. The application of cold produces a transient excitation, and thus causes a preliminary enhancement of suction. Pro- longed application of cold, causing a depression of excit- ability, brings about arrest of suction. As suction is produced by the rhythmic activity of the tissue of the entire plant, local death, as by scalding or application of poison, does not cause its arrest, until the whole plant is killed. When the sum total of the latent energy of the tissue— that is to say, its tonic condition—is below par, its rhythmic suctional activity comes to a stop; fresh application of stimulus, however, renews this activity. Osmotic substances, as regards their stimulatory action, may be either neutral or excitatory. If such a solution be applied at the root, there will in the former case be a diminu- tion, and in the latter, if the excitatory action be relatively great, an increase of suction. The application of poison abolishes local excitability and power of suction. In some cases this arrest of suction may occur quickly. But the total abolition of suction by poison only takes place, for reasons already explained, on the death of the plant as a whole. CELAPT EAR | XOCX THE PHENOMENON OF PROPULSION OF SAP AND ITS VARIOUS EFFECTS The mechanics of the ascent of sap: (a) Uni-directioned flow—(é4) Initiation of multiple rhythmic excitations—Connection between conduction of excitation and conduction of sap—Rapidity of ascent of sap accounted for by stimulatory action — Positive and negative pressures due to one cause—(I) Positive pressure —(2) Negative pressure—(3) Irregular variations of pressure—Direct con- duction and conduction by relays— Excretion of water—Excretion of nectar— Translocation of organic food-substances—Mechanical response to suctional activity —Effect of warmth—Effect of cold—Explanation of the drooping of leaves during frost—Explanation of response and recovery—Antagonistic actions of internal energy and external stimulus. THERE are various phenomena connected with the transport of water in plants, which are at present considered as entirely distinct. Thus, for example, when a plant is cut above the root, the exuding water exerts a considerable positive pressure on a manometer, this being known as root or exudation pressure. But when manometers are inserted in lateral holes bored in the trunk of a tree, a negative pressure is observed. These facts have led to the inference that there exist, in a transpiring plant, two independent forces, one of suction, and the other of pressure. The negative pressure, again, which is, generally speaking, maximal at the top ofa tree, falls to a minimal value near the root. But this fall is characterised by very irregular fluctuations, the negative pressure, in a zone below, being sometimes greater than that at a given distance above. I shall now proceed to show, however, that these very various results are not actually due, as supposed, to the operation of distinct forces, but are, on the contrary, so many different effects, under different conditions, of the rhythmic 390 PLANT RESPONSE cellular activity of the plant-tissue. I shall show that this activity is sufficient to account not only for the phenomena of the excretion of solution from pores and nectaries, but also for other phenomena, whose connection with it has been little suspected. The mechanics of the ascent of sap.—We have seen that throughout the plant there are active rhythmic tissues, which by their excitatory activity bring about the movement of water. We have next to inquire, therefore, as to how this excitatory action is initiated, and further in what manner so many activities in different zones of the stem are correlated, so as to give rise to a uni-directional flow, generally upwards ; for the rhythmic activity which may cause any given group of cells to act as a pump, would not alone be sufficient to account for the regulated, one-directioned flow of sap, since, while one such group propels water in one direction, there is no obvious reason why another should not propel it in the opposite. (a) Uni-directioned flow.—tIn connection with the trans- mission of multiple excitatory waves, such, for example, as those which we have seen in Azophytum, it is important to note that these repeated excitations are all initiated at the original point of stimulation, and are propagated in proper sequence from point to point outwards. Thus, after each wave of excitation is exhausted, it starts anew from the original point. This sequence of multiple excitations is exactly parallel to that which is observed in multi-ciliated tissues in which the cilia repeatedly contract in sequence. Thus a single cilium at one end gives, as it were, a signal which is followed serially by the rest. This being so, it is clear that if such a multi-ciliated tissue take the form of a hollow tube, with the ciliated surface inwards, and if the tube be filled with water, then, owing to this peculiarity of the multiply-responding cilia, the water will always be driven in one direction. A somewhat similar phenomenon occurs in the blood- circulation of animals, where the s¢zus giving the signal, the rhythmic contraction of the PROPULSION OF SAP AND ITS VARIOUS EFFECTS 391 heart proceeds towards the ventricle, and the pumping-action thus initiated produces a one-directioned flow of fluid. In the leaf of Azophytum, strongly excited at, say, the inner end, we have similar rhythmic excitations, passing in regular succession from leaflet to leaflet, the innermost leaflet, which is near the seat of multiple excitation, giving the signal to the rest. And just as rigid sequence is observable in the movements of motile cilia, so it is also seen in the plant, by the depression in orderly series of its lateral motile leaflets. (6) Initzation of multiple rhythmic excitations—We have just seen that in multiple rhythmic response, the multiple waves of excitation proceed serially outwards, from the point of excitation. We also saw in Chapter XXI. that the wave of excitatory contraction is attended by the propulsion of water in the direction of propagation of excitation. It remains to be determined, then, with regard to the ascent of sap in a plant, how this one-directioned propagation of excitation is initiated. In the case of an intact plant, the root is acted on con- stantly by various forms of stimulation, among which are (1) contact with the soil; (2) friction of the growing organ against rough surfaces; (3) turgor of its own tissues, due to absorption of water; and there may also be in addition, stimulation by (4), chemical substances of various kinds in the soil, which possibly exert some excitatory influence. All these factors, separately or in combination, serve to set up multiple rhythmic excitation at the extremity of the plant, and the excitatory effect is transmitted upwards, preferably along certain better-conducting tissues. Similarly, in the case of a cut branch placed ‘in water, rhythmic excitation is initiated at the lower end by excessive turgor, just as we found rhythmic movements to be initiated in Desmodium leaflets at standstill, by the artificial increase of internal hydro- static pressure, that is to say, by excessive turgor (p. 348). Connection between conduction of excitation and con- duction of sap.—It was said in the case of the intact plant 392 PLANT RESPONSE that excitation is transmitted upwards by conducting tissues and we have already seen, in studying transmission of excita- tion (p. 250), that the fibro-vascular elements are those which conduct best. It is therefore to be expected that the move- ment of water, which is itself an excitatory effect, should also follow by preference the length of the fibro-vascular elements ; and it is here worthy of note that the ascent of sap is known to take place preferably along such channels. Again, I have shown (p. 250) that the power of conducting excitation is greater along the length of the plant than across ; and we find also that the same is true as regards the transport of water, which is known to be greater in the direc- tion of length. Conduction of excitation takes place with very great slowness across parenchymatous tissues, and the same is the case as regards the conduction of water through such a tissue. If, further,a plant were to be excited from above, instead of below, the transmission of excitation would be downwards, instead of upwards. We might then conceive of the possibility of reversing the direction of the normal transport of water. In such a case, rhythmic excitation would need to be initiated at the top, instead of at the bottom, of the plant. This may be seen in the experiment already referred to, of placing a leafy branch upside down, with its leaves in water. Rhythmic activity due to excessive turgor being now initiated at the upper extremity of the branch, the water- movement is reversed, and sap exudes from the cut end of the stem. Turning back, however, to the subject of the trans- mission of excitation, we have found, it will be remembered, in the case of Bzophytum that this takes place with greater rapidity in a centrifugal than in a centripetal direction, that is to say, in the direction of the normal transport of water. It is therefore interesting to note that a reversed water- movement is, in general, known to be somewhat less rapid than the normal flow. Rapidity of ascent of sap accounted for by stimula- tory action.—One great difficulty with regard to the ascent of sap has lain in its relatively great rapidity. No theory PROPULSION OF SAP AND ITS VARIOUS EFFECTS 393 hitherto suggested has been held to account for this. As, however, we now find that the ascent of sap is due to the propulsive energy of vigorous excitatory contraction pro- ceeding from cell to cell, the rapidity of the movement is easily understood. We thus see how by the action of this cellular machinery, set in motion by stimulus, an upward movement of water takes place. We have in fact an active chain of pumps, working throughout the length of the plant, partly carrying water themselves, and partly pumping it into the better con- ducting vessels of the xylem; and there is no limit to the height to which it may, by such means, be lifted. Positive and negative pressures due to one cause.— Let us suppose an india-rubber pipe, open at its upper end, and provided throughout its length with a series of pumps, one above the other, each of these being independently engaged in raising water upwards. The individual activity of these several pumps may or may not be uniform, but, provided that they are sufficiently numerous, when the pipe is placed in connection with a supply of water, the result of their com- bined action will be that water will be sucked in at the lower end, and ejected at the upper, in a uniform stream. If now we confined our attention to the lowermost pump, it would appear to us to be /forvceng water up ; if, on the other hand, we observed the uppermost pump alone, it would appear to be sucking water up; and if, finally, we selected some intermediate point for scrutiny, we should discover that the pumps above were sucking, and those below pressing water upwards. Thus, one single effect, namely, the rhythmic activity of pumps, is made to appear various, by simply changing the point of view. Again, certain peculiarities of variation of pressure may appear in the pipe as a whole or in particular parts of it, depending on the rates of supply and removal of water. (1) Positive pressure.—-We may now suppose the aperture of escape at the upper end to be narrowed. The water pumped into the flexible pipe being thus in a state of . 394 PLANT RESPONSE compression, will produce a bulging or over-turgidity, and a manometric tube inserted laterally will indicate a positive pressure. Or if, under these circumstances, we make a cut in the pipe, we shall observe exudation under pressure. Now, this corresponds to the exudation pressure, causing so-called ‘bleeding, which we find on making incisions in plants in spring time, when the loss of water by transpiration is feeble, the buds being still unfolded; or even in summer, if trans- piration be by any means prevented. In connection with this exudation of sap, we have to remember that the whole question is one of income and ex- penditure. Generally speaking, the loss by transpiration is less at the end of winter or the beginning of spring than in other seasons. Thus the wild date palm, or Phanzx sylvestris, yields considerable quantities of sugary sap from incisions in the stem, at the end of winter or the beginning of spring. In the case of ‘the Palmyra palm (Borassus flabelliformis) of Bengal, however, the increase of cellular activity in summer more than compensates for the loss by transpiration, and sugary juice is collected in the height of summer at the top of the tree by incisions in the peduncle. The pressure exerted by the sap may be gathered from the fact that these trees are often more than a hundred feet high. (2) Negative pressure.—We may next suppose that in the chain of pumps, those at the upper end are the most active, and the aperture of escape wide open, the removal of water being further aided by evaporation. The loss of water being thus greater than the supply, it is clear that there will be a negative pressure in the pipe, and the mercury in a testing lateral manometer will be sucked in. This corresponds to the negative pressure exhibited by an actively transpiring plant. In such a case it will be noted that the activity of the rhythmic cells, which is the fundamental cause of the ascent of sap, is further aided by the evaporation from the leaves. Concentration of cell-sap also, and the osmotic action thereby produced, may then constitute an additional auxiliary factor. PROPULSION OF SAP AND ITS VARIOUS EFFECTS 395 When the stem ofan actively transpiring plant is cut across, the stump of the plant does not always immediately show bleeding, but often, on the contrary, will suck in water. This is generally ascribed solely to the existence of negative pres- sure in the stem. There is another element in the problem, however, which is generally overlooked. By such stimulus as that of amputation, excitation must be produced at the cut end of the stem, which is propagated downwards, and tends to induce a reversal of flow. But when the negative tension and the excitatory effect, due to stimulus of cut, have both subsided, exudation will begin to take place. (3) Irregular variations of pressure._-It is evident from what has been said that the hydrostatic pressure at any given zone of the tissue will depend on the relative activities of cells below and cells above. Cells immediately above, by their activity, produce suction or negative pressure; and those immediately below, an increase or positive pressure. The resultant pressure at any individual zone depends, therefore, on the algebraical summation of these. Now, though all the cells throughout the length of the plant may be active, yet there will be.some difference in their activities. Or the same cells again, at different periods of life and different conditions, may undergo variations of their activity. There are, in nature, many disturbing influences which produce local variations of excitability ; hence, by the dis- tribution of cells whose excitabilities are irregular, we may obtain a variation of internal pressure from the top to the bottom of the plant, which is not uniform but fluctuating. Direct conduction and conduction by relays.—I have already said that the movement of water, being mainly brought about by excitatory reactions, will take place preferentially along conducting channels. We have seen that every tissue: possesses the power of conduction to a greater or less degree ; in parenchymatous cells, however, owing to the presence of numerous more or less complete septa, trans- mission is enfeebled, whereas in prosenchymatous fibro- vascular elements it is very much facilitated. Thus, for 396 PLANT RESPONSE conduction through long tracts, the fibro-vascular tissues are the most favourable. But even in this case the transmission of the excitatory effect may be much enfeebled by distance. Or an interposition of parenchymatous elements may offer a relative obstruction to the transmission. In such cases there might be pseudo-conduction by means of ‘relays.’ For as an example of the last case we may imagine a mass of excitable parenchymatous tissue, against which a conducting tissue abuts. This mass, being supplied with water by the conduct- ing elements, may become over-turgid, and thus rhythmic activity may be initiated in it de novo. That rhythmic activity may under favourable circum- stances be started locally in a mass of excitable tissue, we have seen in the case of the pulvinus of Desmodium. For the fact that activity in this case was not due to any transmitted impulse was proved in the experiment on the localisation of the excitable area, when it was found that an isolated leaflet, if sufficiently turgid, could pulsate. Under natural condi- tions, the necessary turgidity is maintained by the cellular activity of the tissue below. It is worth while to remember, in this regard, ihat the characteristic pulsation of the leaflet has no immediate connection with that rhythmic activity of the plant-tissue which brings about the ascent of sap. The period of pulsation of the leaflet is determined by certain constants of its cell-complex. We may in fact have various vibration-periods in different organs of the same plant, the different oscillations being brought about by the turgidity caused by the ascent of the sap, just as the same electrical current may give rise to various frequencies of vibration of different electro-magnetic vibrators included in the same circuit. Excretion of water.—The rhythmic activity of a mass of excitable cells is seen again in the case of such as ac- tively excrete water. A very striking example is that of Colocasia esculentum, in which the successive expulsions of water-drops noticed by Musset were as many as eighty-five in one minute. PROPULSION OF SAP AND ITS VARIOUS EFFECTS 397 A distinction is sometimes made between this excretion— due to the special local activity of a certain group of cells— and the somewhat passive excretion of water from water- pores, which is said to be caused by the general pressure of exudation. But this difference is really one of degree and not of kind. All cells are excitable, and the exudation pressure itself is produced by cellular activity. As regards excitability, however, we may have a transition from moderately to highly excitable cells distributed in a continu- ous or discontinuous manner. Even in the stem we have seen that there are cases of irregular distribution, bringing about irregularities of water-pressure. Extreme instances of these are found in Desmodium, and in the actively excreting cells of Colocasza, where highly excitable cells are localised in special areas. A test which is sometimes insisted on as a means of distinguishing between the active and so-called pas- sive excretions is, that in the latter case the flow ceases from the excreting organ, as soon as the branch is cut off. But this is not by any means a satisfactory proof of the absence of active excreting cells in the latter. We saw that the only distinction between the activities of the multiply-responding tissues of Bzophytum and Desmodium, lay in the fact that the latter had the capacity to hold latent a large amount of energy by which the rhythmic activity was maintained even on the cessation of a directly exciting cause. In the case of Desmodium, indeed, if the tonic condition be above par, and the leaf as a whole be cut off and isolated, without any supply of water, the rhythmic activity will be maintained for a con- siderable time, though the turgidity is undergoing constant diminution. But when the tonic condition of the plant is below par, its rhythmic activity comes to a stop, and can only be maintained by an artificial increase of internal hydrostatic pressure. Similarly in the case of water-excreting organs, we have some which under a favourable condition can maintain their activity for a considerable period, even when the supply of water is cut off, while in other instances activity can be maintained only under favourable conditions of turgidity. 398 PLANT RESPONSE Excretion of nectar.—It is often assumed that the excretion of nectar is due to plasmolytic action. The excreted solution of sugar dries up by evaporation, and this by plasmolysis draws up more from behind. We have seen that the ascent of sap in the plant is brought about mainly by rhythmic activity, and that concentrated solutions in the leaves may help this movement osmotically. In the case of nectaries, the presence of concentrated sugar solution outside may thus help continued excretion, but this does not explain the initiation of excretion, which could only have been caused by the rhythmic activity of cells. Translocation of organic food-substances.—Though the flow of organic food-materials towards places where there is a deficit may be brought about by diosmosis, yet such a mode of diffusion must be extremely slow. A more rapid transport than could be produced by this means would appear to be a necessity. There are certain considerations moreover which may be brought forward, tending to show that this translocation of food-materials receives considerable aid from stimulatory actions. Czapek’s observation, too, that there is a cessation of translocation in a chloroformed leaf-stalk, points to the inference that it is a physiological process. We know that an excitatory action proceeds from the more to the less stimulated. Now the accumulation of a large quantity of organic food-material may of itself act as a stimulating agent in a given case; thus an excitatory move- ment would proceed from cell to cell, from places where there was excess to places where there was deficit. In this way large quantities of food-materials may be rapidly transported by excitatory reaction, through conducting channels. And such transport is also possible by stimulatory action even through unspecialised cells. These ordinary cells are known to be connected with each other, by means of pores and plasmic threads. Mr. Horace Brown has shown (Phzd. Trans. vol. cxciii.) that transport of fluid may take place through such a ‘multi-perforate septum’ with almost as great rapidity PROPULSION OF SAP AND ITS VARIOUS EFFECTS 399 as if no closing membrane were present. It is clear that by the contracting expulsive action of excited cells, transport of materials might be brought about more rapidly than by mere osmotic action. Mechanical response to suctional activity.—The effect of that internal rhythmic activity which, as we have seen, is caused by the absorption of energy from various forms of stimulus, is to induce an increase of turgidity throughout the plant. I shall next proceed to describe a mechanical means of obtaining some indication of this internal activity. I have shown that owing to the dorsi-ventral differentiation in the pulvinus of J/zosa, when the turgidity of the organ is increased, the leaf is erected; and when the turgidity is decreased, as by the direct action of an external stimulus, the leaf is depressed. Similar effects occur not in pulvinated organs alone, but in all dorsi-ventral organs, in which the lower half is more excitable than the upper. Thus, for example, the lower half of the petiole carrying the lateral leaflets of Bzophytum is more excitable than the upper half. It is true that the leaf possesses a slightly developed pulvinus ; the differential excitability on which the motile response depends is not, however, confined to this pulvinus alone, but extends throughout the length of the petiole. The petiole thus acts like a diffuse pulvinoid. The action of stimulus on the diffuse pulvinoid of Biophytum and on the pulvinus of JZzmosa is to produce the fall of the leaf. And just as the leaf of M/zmosa was erected by increased turgidity, so in Bzophytum also we should expect to have, with similar increase of turgidity, a similar responsive movement of erection ; and this I find to be the case. The erection of the leaf of J/zmosa or Biophytum is therefore a mechanical indication of an increase of turgidity or of positive turgidity- variation, by whatever means this may have been induced. Such an increase may be brought about by an augmentation of suctional activity. And the up movements of dorsi-ventral 400 PLANT RESPONSE petioles in which the lower surface is the more excitable may thus be taken as mechanical indications of such added activity. Effect of warmth.—We have already seen that applica- tion of warm water to the root of a plant, increases its suctional activity. I was able to demonstrate this fact by means of the mechanical response of Mzmosa and Biophytum. On making the application, the leaves of both plants responded by erec- tion to a position which in the case of Mzmosa was 9 mm. and in the case of Bzophytum 12 mm. above normal. A rise of temperature we have also seen to have the effect of enhanc- ing the internal energy of the plant. By doing this, it brings about an erection of the leaves. Effect of cold.—We have seen, on the other hand, that the effect of cold is to stop rhythmic activity, and thereby to arrest the ascent of sap. The motile indication given by the leaf would in this case be the opposite to that of positive turgidity-variation, that is to say, would consist of a fall or droop. On applying ice-cold water to the root of Mzmosa I found that the effect of cooling was to induce a lowering or drooping of the leaf, to a position of 3 mm. below the normal. In Bzophytum the corresponding fall was through 5 mm. Lowering of temperature, by depressing the internal energy of the plant, has the same effect. Explanation of the drooping of leaves during frost.— These experiments offer an explanation of that drooping of leaves which is observed in frost, and of the disappearance of this drooping when a plant is restored to a warmer atmosphere indoors ; for we have seen that when the internal energy of the plant is normal, or above par, its suctional activity and consequent turgidity are high, this favourable internal condi- tion being outwardly exhibited by the erection of the leaves. But when the internal energy is below par, the reverse effect is seen in their droop or fall. Explanation of response and recovery.-—_It has been stated, when describing the effect of stimulus in inducing a fall of the leaf, and its subsequent erection, that the latter PROPULSION OF SAP AND ITS VARIOUS EFFECTS 401 movement, namely, that of recovery, was an active and not a passive process. Certain experiments that I am about to describe will enable us to analyse response and _ recovery still more closely. When an organ is locally excited, the contraction induced results in an expulsion of water, thus reducing the turgidity of the organ, and the consequence of this is a mechanical fall of the leaf. Some internal activity now forces the water back into the organ from which it was expelled; and we have the recovery or erection of the leaf. That it is the internal energy of the plant to which this recovery is due, will be seen clearly from the following experiment. A leaf of Bzophytum falls, as has been said before, when it is stimulated, whatever be the form of stimulus. I shall explain in my chapter on the effect of Fic. 166. Record showing Recovery to be Hastened by the Increase of Internal Activity which is caused by Application of Warm Water to the Roots The first part of the record shows the fall of the leaf, due to direct stimulation by stimulus of light, that the effect of photic stimulus is the same as that of any other form, while its special advantage is that sunlight during thirty minutes. Stimulus is next cut off at point marked by interruption of record. After-effect persists for five minutes, and there is a subsequent slow recovery. Warm water applied to root at moment x, with the result of quickening the rate of recovery by fifteen times. it may be applied without causing any mechanical disturbance, and that its intensity can be very easily regulated. On subjecting a petiole of Azophytum, therefore, to the action of sunlight, the leaf responded by depression, the average rate of fall of the tip of the leaf being -16 mm. per minute. Inhalf an hour it had passed through almost 5 mm. On now shutting off the light, the after-effect persisted for another five minutes, when there followed recovery by slow DD 402 PLANT RESPONSE erection of the leaf at a rate of :12 mm. per minute. Warm water was now poured on the root, thus suddenly increasing the internal activity of the plant. Now; if it be true that recovery is brought about by this factor of internal energy, then the increase of internal energy ought to produce a sudden augmentation of the rate of the recovery. That this is the case will be seen from the record (fig. 166), where it will be noticed that the enhanced rate of recovery from this point is 1°8 mm. per minute, that is to say, fifteen times the normal rate. Antagonistic actions of internal energy and external stimulus.—We thus see that it is the internal energy of the plant—vaguely known as a favourable tonic condition— which actively determines the recovery of the organ. This will explain the fact which I have mentioned elsewhere, that in summer, when the internal energy is considerable, the leaf of Mimosa recovers from the effect of stimulus in about six minutes, whereas in winter, when the internal energy is low, the same process may take as long as eighteen minutes. We thus see that as regards mechanical response the external stimulus and internal energy act antagonistically. Local external stimulus induces a diminution of turgidity, while internal energy causes an increase of turgidity. Thus when the internal turgidity is very great it opposes the mechanical response to external stimulus. This we saw in the case of over-turgid leaves of JM/zmosa, and in those of Artocarpus during the rainy season, which, though excited, did not exhibit mechanical response to stimulation (pp. 49, 58). This will be clearly understood also from an attentive consideration of the experiment, the record of which is given in fig. 166. In that case, had the warm water which in- creased the internal activity been applied earlier at the root, that is to say during the application of external stimulus, the induced internal turgidity would then have been so great as to arrest the responsive down movement of the leaf. The amplitude of the response to external stimulus would thus have undergone diminution or even abolition. PROPULSION OF SAP AND ITS VARIOUS EFFECTS 403 We must remember, however, that the internal energy which maintains the normal turgid condition is itself the result of energy previously absorbed from external sources ; and if the plant be cut off from these sources of external energy, then its own tonic condition will fall below par. Thus the leaves of many plants are seen to droop when kept too long in darkness, and exposure to light makes them recover their natural position of normal turgidity. Hencein the case of a plant whose condition is sub-tonic the leaves may be made turgid by exposure to light ; but after the attainment of the normal tonic condition, exposure to strong light will bring about the proper contractile response to stimulus, with the gharacteristic external motile indication of diminished turgidity. We have thus seen the various effects produced by the internal or latent energy of the plant. We have seen it bring about the ascent of sap by means of the increased activity of the plant-cells. It was seen to produce exudation pressure, and excretion of nectar from intact plants. We have traced it out to its appropriate expression by the lateral movements characteristic of positive turgidity-variation, in the case of anisotropic or dorsi-ventral organs. We have seen, too, how necessary it is to the production of recovery of an organ from the action of an external stimulus, the effects of local ex- ternal stimulation, and of this internal activity, being opposed in character. Any increase of this internal energy is thus a factor tending to hasten the recovery of the organ from stimulation, and when it is sufficiently great, it may, by its antagonistic action, reduce the amplitude or even abolish response to external local stimulus. We have again seen this internal activity finding motile expression in the auto- nomous movements of leaflets of Desmodium; but, for its mechanical exhibition, we need not confine our attention to the sensitive plants so called, for we shall find the same internal activity exhibited mechanically by all plants, in their rhythmic growth-responses, to be described in the following chapters, DD2 404 PLANT RESPONSE SUMMARY The ascent of sap is fundamentally due to excitatory reaction, its uni-directioned flow being brought about by the passage, from point to point, of the co-ordinated excitatory reaction, propelling water forward. This rhythmic excitation is initiated in the intact plant at its root, by stimulus of contact with soil, the friction of the growing organ against rough surfaces, the excessive tur- gidity caused by the absorption of water, and possibly by the chemical stimulus of substances present in the soil. In’ the case of cut branches placed in water, the excessive turgidity at the cut end initiates rhythmic activity. Again, if stimulus be applied at the top instead of at the root, the direction of water-conduction may be reversed, along with the reversal of propagation of excitation. The following facts show the intimate relation between the conduction of stimulus and conduction of water : (a2) The movement of water takes place preferentially through the fibro-vascular elements, these being also the better conductors of excitation. (6) The conduction of excitation along a plant is greater than across. The same is true of its power of transport of water. (c) Though conduction of excitation may take place either upwards or downwards, yet there is a_ preferential direction for such conduction. The same is true of the trans- port of water. The same movement of water produced by the co- ordinated rhythmic activity of cells throughout the plant appears as cither suctional or pressure movement, according to the point of view. When the removal of water from the plant is in any way arrested, a positive pressure is produced, owing to the exces- sive accumulation of water. When, on the other hand, the PROPULSION OF SAP AND ITS VARIOUS EFFECTS 405 loss of water by transpiration is greater than the supply, a negative pressure will be observed. The ascent of sap primarily due to cellular activity, may be secondarily aided by evaporation from the leaves, and the osmotic action of the concentrated cell sap in the leaves. Owing to the distribution of unequally active cells, an irregular variation of pressure is induced in the stem. The excitatory movement may be transmitted to a dis- tance by conduction, or there may be conduction by ‘relays.’ An isolated mass of highly excitable tissue may thus be excited de novo. The excretion of water and of nectar are phenomena of cellular activity, analogous to that which brings about the ascent of sap. The translocation of food-material is also probably due, at least in part, to excitatory reaction. The internal activity of the plant, causing increase of turgidity, may be detected mechanically by that erection of the leaf which is characteristic of the positive turgidity- variation. Any increase of internal activity is exhibited in dorsi- ventral organs, such as the petioles of Wimosa, Biophytum, and Artocarpus, by the erection of the leaf. Thus, when the internal energy of the plant is increased by a rise of tem- perature, the leaves become erected. Conversely, under the action of cold, on account of the diminution of the latent energy, the opposite effect, or droop, is induced. This explains the drooping of various leaves during frost, and their subsequent erection when brought into a warmer, atmo- sphere. This internal energy is also an important factor in bringing about the recovery of an organ from the effect of external local stimulus. The effect of external local stimulus in causing the diminution of turgidity of an organ is thus antagonised by the internal activity, which causes an increase of turgidity. 406 PLANT RESPONSE The internal energy, when sufficiently great, may thus hasten the recovery of the organ from the effect of stimulus. This increased internal energy may also reduce the am- plitude, culminating in the total abolition of mechanical response, as seen in over-turgid A7zmosa, or in Azstocarpus during the rains. PAREOVI. GROW ld CHAPTER XXXI THE RECORD OF GROWTH-RESPONSE The simple Growth-Recorder—The Balanced Growth-Recorder—Rhythmic growth-response—-Growth-response and excitatory response—Law of direct and indirect effects of excitation—Positive turgidity-variation as indirect effect of excitation—Mechanical test—Significance of ‘ inner stimuli.’ ONE of the most characteristic manifestations of life is growth. The question then arises, whether this particular manifestation is to be regarded as a distinct and specific phenomenon, unlike all others, or whether it may be possible to trace a connection between it and those responsive re- actions with which we are already familiar. The occurrence, in response to stimulus, of numerous growth-curvatures, sometimes positive and sometimes nega- tive in character, offers us again a problem of very great complexity. It is sometimes supposed that stimulus retards, and sometimes that it accelerates, growth. But it is difficult to understand how the same influence can produce opposite effects.. Then, again, there is intruded upon :the problem the unknown effect of ‘inner stimuli.’ From all these it will be seen that the subject of growth and growth-movements is one of extreme obscurity, and that the difficulties which baffle us can only be met satisfactorily if we are able to analyse and follow out, one by one, the various elements that enter into the problem. We have seen in the Desmodium \eaflet at standstill, and in that of Bzophytum under ordinary circumstances, that when the latent energy is not excessive, we obtain a single movement in response to a single stimulus. When the sum total of the latent energy of the tissue, however, is above par, 410 PLANT RESPONSE it is manifested in a rhythmic manner, by periodic variations of turgidity, bringing on responsive movements. It was also stated that responsive movements under the action of stimulus took place in ordinary young tissues, these move- ments being lateral when the tissue was anisotropic, and longitudinal when it was strictly radial. It would follow, then, that when the sum total of the latent energy in such tissues was above par, they might be expected—like the leaflet of Bzophytum or Desmodium under similar conditions— to exhibit their rhythmic excitation by repeated lateral or longitudinal movements. I shall now proceed to show that in the case of these young tissues, under favourable circum- stances, this multiple rhythmic excitation finds expression in the responsive movement known as growth. In the majority of instances an organ is not absolutely radial; hence, during growth, we obtain the lateral respon- sive movements which are known as circumnutation. In bilateral growing organs, these movements are to and fro, in strict parallelism to the to and fro movements of the leaflet of Azophytum. In such instances the axis of bilate- rality is fixed, and the responsive movement takes place in a definite plane. In the leaflet of Desmodium also rectilinear movements are often observed ; but as a rule, owing to the revolution of the bilateral axis, the movement of the leaflet is circular or elliptical, and in the case of growing organs, from the same cause, circular or elliptical movements of nutation are common. The ideally simple and most interesting example of this multiple rhythmic activity is seen, however, in the growth-movements of radial organs, these being longi- tudinal ; for there is not in this case that complication which arises from the gradual shifting of the bilateral axis seen in the growth of anisotropic organs. In order to prove the identity of these rhythmic growth- movements with multiple response, we have to show (1) that such rhythm is a characteristic of growth; (2) that each pulsatory growth-movement of the series exhibits all the characteristics of true response ; (3) that the series itself THE RECORD OF GROWTH-RESPONSE 411 is characterised by the same cyclic variation which we have observed in the case of multiple response ; (4) that just as the application of appropriate stimulus renews pulsation in a _ Desmodium at standstill, so, in a plant with growth at stand- still, appropriate stimulation renews pulsatory growth ; and, lastly (5), that the modifying influence of external agents is similar in both cases. From a series of observations, taken at intervals of several minutes, on Spyrogyra princeps, Hofmeister found that growth undergoes fluctuation, the first and second maximal points in his series of observations being separated by an interval of forty-four minutes, and the second and third by an interval of ninety-five minutes. Such experiments, however, have laboured under the great disadvantage of discontinuity, and in order to overcome this I undertook to devise some appa- ratus by whose means growth-pulsations might be recorded continuously, in such a way as to give not only the period, but also the individual peculiarities of each pulsation. And I may here forestall matters to say that by such means I have been able to detect longitudinal pulsations two hundred times as quick as those observed by Hofmeister. Conditions to be kept in view.—Before attempting to demonstrate the pulsatory character of growth-movements, however, I shall point out certain facts which it is essential to remember. We have seen that under rapidly succeeding excitations, the separate responsive effects become merged, and a response is produced, which is apparently continuous, although the stimuli themselves were discontinuous. This is seen, for instance, in the first part of the tetanic curve (figs. 49, 50). But when once the maximum responsive effect is produced, as there can be no further additive effect, the subsequent responses show themselves in a series of fluctuations of the top of the tetanic curve. In that form of response, which we are now considering, as there is no maximum limit, the additive effect of growth continues indefinitely. It is thus clear that when rhythmic excitation is very rapid, it may produce a growth-movement which 412 PLANT RESPONSE appears to be continuous. But when this rapidity is not excessive, it should be possible, by employing sufficient magnification and a suitably quick rate of movement of the recording surface, to display its actual pulsatory cha- racter. The sensitiveness of this mode of detection becomes again very much increased if we employ the method of balance, or compensation, which will be described presently. We thus require a high magnification, and some means of continuous record. aie A high magnification may be produced by microscopic optical projection, but this labours under the great dis- advantage that the specimen is subjected to the strong and unilateral stimulus of light, by which its normal growth- movements are greatly modified. The ordinary auxano- metric method, again, cannot be employed, (1) because the magnification produced is not sufficiently great; and (2) because the inertia of the wheel, and the unavoidable friction of the apparatus, themselves combine to obliterate the quick pulsations of the growth-response. The Crescograph.—All these difficulties were over- come by the use of my Optical Lever, for making growth- records. The lever is made extremely light, and the fulcrum- rod rests on agate planes. When the tip of the growing organ is attached by a thread to the short arm of the Lever, the length of the latter being 5 cm., and when the record- ing surface is at a distance of 2°5 metres, a magnification of 1,000 times is obtained. This is in most cases more than sufficient. But, when necessary, a magnification of 10,000 times can easily be secured. In order to avoid any dis- turbance due to vibration in the room, the apparatus is supported on a steady bracket, fixed on the wall. Using these ordinary precautions, records are obtained with this instrument which are absolutely free from external disturb- ance. The Balanced Crescograph.—In records of growth we obtain a sloping curve whose abscissa represents time ; and ordinate, the elongation produced in the organ during THE RECORD OF GROWTH-RESPONSE 413 that time. A variation of growth will produce a variation in the slope of the curve. When this variation of growth, however, is slight, the variation of slope of the curve is so small as not to be detected. It was in order to detect and measure such small variations that I devised the Method of Balance, in which the average rate of growth is represented by the horizontal line of balance, any fluctuation, even the slightest, appearing as a deviation from this horizontal. The influence of various agencies, again, may be displayed in a marked manner, by using this method ; for in such cases we do not so much require the rate of growth itself, as the vartation—t.e. acceleration or retardation—in the normal rate, which is induced by one agent or another. The principle of the Method of Balance consists in making the spot of light—which is moving in response to growth—become stationary, by subjecting it to a compen- sating movement. An example will make this clear. We shall suppose the average rate of growth to be 1:'2 mm. per hour. This will cause an excursion of the moving spot of light, from, say, left to right, through 1,200 mm. by the end of the hour, in that case where the magnification is 1,000. Had the growth been uniform, this would have meant a movement of 20 mm. per minute. But if not uniform, the rate might sometimes have risen above, and at others fallen below, this average. If now we subject the spot of light to a uniform compensating movement, such as by itself would have made it move from right to left of the recording sur- face, to the extent of 1,200 mm. by the end of the hour, we shall find that, being acted on by these two opposite move- ments, of growth and compensation, the spot will remain approximately on a single base line of compensation. The fluctuations, or variations, which have occurred in this average rate of growth, will, however, be recorded as deviations to one side or other of this mean neutral line. Thus it will be seen that the slightest deviation from a uniform rate of growth, will be found displayed by the record of the moving spot of light. We are further enabled, from our 414 PLANT RESPONSE knowledge of the speed of the recording-drum, and the balancing rate, and from an inspection of the curve itself, to determine not only the periodicities, but also the absolute value of the rate of variation of growth, at any given moment. The compensating movement to which I have referred is effected by means of an hydraulic device. The spot of light from the Optical Lever falls upon a mirror attached to a second lever, or to a rotating wheel. The arm of the lever, or a thread which is passed round _ the wheel, is attached to a float on the surface of a cylinder of water. Water is escaping from this cylinder, by means of a syphon arrangement, at arate which can be adjusted with the — greatest = nicety. The float can Fic. 167. Diagrammatic Representation of thus be made to de- Balanced Crescograph p, plant attached to Optic Lever, L, with mirror, scend at aa speed M, attached to elcid) reste La Pilg that is desired, this Sra ste aces as waaeied to its ful. descent producing a crum-rod ; B, balancing wheel adjusting differ- sotation of the second ences of level of the two limbs of syphon, s. lever or of the wheel. We have, then, two mirrors, of which one is rotated in one direction by the growth-movement of the plant, and the second in the opposite direction by the descent of the float. A spot of light reflected on the two mirrors will thus remain stationary when the precise balance is effected, by proper regulation of the escape of water from the cylinder (fig. 167). THE RECORD OF GROWTH-RESPONSE 415 This outflow of water is roughly adjusted by opening the stop-cock, to a greater or less extent. The finer adjustment is then effected by the suitable variation of the difference of level in the two limbs of the syphon. One end is con- nected with a flexible india-rubber tube, which is attached to a string passing over a pulley, and fixed to the adjustable wheel B, attached to the observer’s table. Rotation of the wheel in one direction will depress this end of the syphon, so increasing the flow, and in the other direction will raise it, so diminishing the escape of water. It will be noticed that the rate of outflow of the water is not in any way affected by the variation of level of the water in the cylinder. It simply depends on the difference of level between the two ends of the syphon. The rate of descent of the float is thus regulated with the utmost nicety, till an absolute balance is obtained. The observer recognises the condition of balance when there is no drifting of the spot of light on the recording drum. The adjusting wheel is now fixed at this position of balance. If any agent should induce an acceleration of growth, the balance is disturbed, and the spot of light moves, say, to the right, or in a positive direction. Any agent which induces retardation will, on the other hand, cause a deflection of the spot of light in a negative direction. Should there be any natural fluctuations in the growth, the oscillation of the spot of light will give an indication of the fact. The record is made in the usual manner on a revolving drum. Fig. 168 illustrates the complete apparatus, which enables us to obtain a record under balanced—or by closing the stop-cock of the syphon, also under unbalanced—con- ditions. The wheel is graduated, and the absolute value of the compensatory movement, at any position of the circular scale, can be previously calibrated, by fixing the plant-mirror, and observing the extent of movement of the spot of light on the drum, due to the subsidence of the float in a given time. For the exhibition of pure longitudinal growth-response, the most perfect specimens are the growing radial styles and 416 PLANT RESPONSE stamens of flowers. There are also other organs, which are more or less strictly radial, such as the peduncle and the hypocotyl. But in these latter cases care should be taken that the specimen have been so grown, that its different sides have been subjected to uniform conditions of light or dark- ness ; for one-sided illumination tends to produce anisotropy. A) e. Fic. 168. Complete Apparatus for Crescographic Record under Ordinary and Balanced Conditions With specimens of all the types mentioned, I have obtained multiple growth-responses when the constituent pulsations . were not too rapid. In some cases, indeed, the effects were so marked that they did not even require a balancing arrangement to render them conspicuous. As an instance of this, I shall give a record of the growth-response of a THE RECORD OF GROWTH-RESPONSE 417 vigorously growing peduncle of Cvocuws which brings out in an interesting manner the mechanics of growth (fig. 169). Rhythmic growth-response.—We saw that when the sum total of energy is above par, a tissue becomes self- excitatory in a multiple or rhythmic manner, giving rise to periodic turgidity-variations. There may thus be respon- sive pulsations of increased turgidity, each followed by slow recovery from such excess. With each such pulse, a transient elongation of the growing tissue will be produced, and the succeeding slow recovery will be more or less incomplete. This incompleteness is due to the deposit of material which fixes growth. The irreversible or per- manent growth-effect produced by each pulsation, will thus be measured by the responsive elongation minus the recovery. To. . Fic. 169. Multiple Growth-responses This is well illustrated (Peduncle of Crocus) in fig. 169, where The ordinate represents the extent of responsive elongations in mm. ; the abscissa, time in three separate sets of AH responses are given, taken from a single specimen in the course of the day. Growth, as will be shown, is not uniform throughout the day, but exhibits variation, in consequence of changing conditions, such as that of temperature. But for a short interval of time, the rate of growth under a constant en- vironment, it may be taken as uniform. In the present RE 418 PLANT RESPONSE instance the maximum rate was as high as ‘0035 and the minimum as low as ‘ooIO mm. per minute. Confining our attention to the uppermost of these series (c), we find that the responsive elongation is very quick, and the recovery slow and incomplete. The average period of a single pulse is twenty seconds. The results of series (¢) are given in the following table: TABLE SHOWING PULSATIONS OF GROWTH Number of | Responsive pulse elongation Recovery enere Time I. “0020 mm. ‘OOIO mm. “OOIO mm. 20 seconds TO Ee cneas' 5) poh ena SAE Voors a 22, 2: ‘oo18 ,, 70605) 55 “0013 a5 TOs Gs 4. ee a3 Tereviey “0009 _,, 20/8 tes Total OO4 Tames SOumns It will be seen from this table that the total growth in eighty seconds is 0047 mm., giving an average rate of growth of ‘0035 mm. per minute. Had a magnifying arrangement not been used, this average rate of growth, shown by the dotted base line, would have appeared as con- tinuous growth. By the magnification of the responsive curve, however, we are enabled to see that such a rate is in reality made up of numerous fluctuating growths of which it is an average. Growth-response and excitatory response.—-If we compare these multiple growth-responses with the multiple mechanical responses of Lzophytum (fig. 116), their similarity is at once evident. As in that case, so here also, recovery is not complete, and the series of effects produced is therefore additive in both cases. With Szophytum, however, when the leaflet is depressed to the utmost, a limit is reached ; but in growth there is no such limit, and the summation of effects may go on indefinitely, until senility and death supervene. THE RECORD OF GROWTH-RESPONSE 419 Yet there is a certain difference between these responses. In the case of Bzophytum, the response is due to a sudden diminution, but in growth, to a sudden increase, of turgidity. This might at first sight appear anomalous, but I shall presently show that both are expressions of an excitatory. reaction ; for we have seen that when the responsive organ of Bzophytum is directly excited there is an expulsion of water, and the response is brought about by negative turgidity- variation. This variation we shall for the sake of con- venience distinguish as the direct effect of stimulation. We saw in the last chapter, however, that an increase of the internal energy of a plant gives rise to an opposite re- sponse, that is to say, one characteristic of positive turgidity- variation. The two responsive turgidity-variations, then, both negative and positive, are alike in being expressions of the excitatory reaction, though the negative variation is the effect of external stimulus applied directly to the responding organ, while the positive variation is to be regarded as the effect of the internal energy of the plant. This internal energy may itself have been derived previously by the plant from external sources of stimulation, or the internal energy of a given point may result from the application of a stimulus at a distance. The pumping-in of water by the stimulated root is an example of the latter case ; the cells are thus made tense, and the potential energy of the tissue is raised above par. Again, we may conceive of another interesting instance as follows. When stimulus is applied at a distance the excita- tory expulsion of water gives rise to a wave of increased turgidity, which produces an abnormal positive response, and this positive turgidity-variation we shall designate as ¢he indirect effect of stimulation. The wave of true excitation may in this case reach the organ after that of positive turgidity-variation, and give rise to the direct effect of stimu- lation, that is to say, depression of the leaflet. We may next imagine that the seat of stimulus is at so great a distance from the responding organ, that the transmitted excitation becomes too much enfeebled, by the long tract which has to 420 PLANT RESPONSE be traversed, to produce the excitatory negative response. In this case, nevertheless, the sudden expulsion of water at a distance will give rise to a wave of increased turgidity, which will reach the responding organ, and produce there only that response which is characteristic of positive turgidity- variation. Positive response as indirect effect of excitation.— Numerous experiments have been described exhibiting the negative turgidity-variation as the direct effect of stimulation. I have already described the production of positive turgidity- Fic. 170. Responses of Leaf ¢f Axtocarpus to Thermal Stimulation Thick dots show points of application of stimulation. In @ stimulus was applied near the responsive pulvinoid and gave rise to normal response of fall—here represented as up movement—preceded by pre- liminary erectile twitch. In @ stimulus was applied at a greater distance. The true excitatory effect did not reach the organ, and we obtain positive erectile response of positive turgidity-variation, here represented as down. variation as the indirect effect of stimulation (p. 400). A fuller demonstration of this, by the electrical method, will be found in Chapter XX XVII. _ I shall here give an experiment which establishes the fact by means of mechanical response, In fig. 32 (reproduced in fig. 170, @) was shown a series of normal mechanical responses of negative turgidity-variation obtained from the leaf of Avtocarpus. The conductivity of the petiole in this case is relatively feeble, and these normal responses, preceded by preliminary positive twitches, were obtained when stimulus was applied at a distance of THE RECORD OF GROWTH-RESPONSE 421 3 mm. from the responsive pulvinoid. I then repeated the experiment with the same leaf, but applying stimulus at the greater distance of 5 mm. The responses now consisted of a series of up movements of the leaf, indicative of positive turgidity-variations (fig. 170, 0), the direct effect of stimulus not reaching the organ. Now, turning our attention to the growing organ, we find that the fibro-vascular element, which possesses the power of conduction to a high degree, is not yet fully established in the zone of growth. If, then, contiguous to the growing zone there be a mass of active tissue thrown into a state of rhythmic excitation, it is to be expected that the indirect effect of such stimulation will alone act, and give rise in the region of growth to pulsations of increased turgidity. It will be remembered from the last chapter that water is conducted by preference along the fibro-vascular elements ; and since these strands end below the zone of growth, it is clear that there must be in this region an accumulation of water, and consequent over-turgidity of the tissue ; a condition which is, as we know, sufficient to initiate rhythmic excitation.’ This region, then, acts like the actively excitable tissue of Co/ocasza, which, as we saw, gives rise to spasmodic expulsions of water. In the latter case there is, however, a channel by which the water escapes, thus relieving the pressure on the tissue ; but the growing organ offers only a cul de sac, and the constant repetition of hydrostatic blows thus effects those positive turgidity-variations that are to result in the responsive elongations and incomplete recoveries of the tissue, bringing about growth-movements. ‘Inner stimuli.—It is thus seen that growth represents the indirect effect of stimulus; its motive power residing in the rhythmic activity of the internal tissues of the plant. This rhythmic activity has been shown to be, in its turn, the result of the tonic condition of the plant, that is to say, of the sum total of energy previously absorbed, and held 1 Or the over-turgidity of the growing region may be sufficient of itself to initiate rhythmic activity. 22 PLANT RESPONSE latent in the tissue (p. 314), which we have designated as the internal energy. We have thus succeeded in defining the actual nature of those ‘inner stimuli’ to which the phenomenon of growth is usually vaguely ascribed. From what has been said, it is clear that the responsive peculiarities of the growing region are not fer se in any way different from those of any other excitable tissue, the apparent contrast between negative and positive turgidity- variations—that is to say, between responsive contractions and responsive expansions—having been shown to depend upon the fact that in one case we see the direct, and in the other the indirect, effects of excitation. If this be so, it follows that the direct application of external stimulus to a growing tissue ought to have the normal effect of excitatory contraction. In other words, while the action of the so-called ‘inner stimuli,’ or internal energy, gives rise, as explained above, to responsive expansions, the direct effect of external local stimulation must be the production of responsive contractions, That this is the case, will be shown in the next chapter. SUMMARY Growth is a phenomenon of multiple response. Each of these multiple growth-responses consists of a sudden elongation, due to a pulse of increased turgidity, followed by an incomplete recovery. The difference between elongation and recovery is the irreversible growth-effect. Such responses, when very rapid, appear as continuous. In ordinary excitatory response there is a pulse of diminished, and in growth-response a pulse of increased, turgidity. Both are, however, effects of excitatory reaction. When a tissue is locally excited, it gives a response of negative turgidity-variation, that is to say, of contraction. This is the direct effect of stimulus. When the source of stimulation is behind, and the inter- vening tissue does not conduct excitation, then the excitatory expulsion of water finds expression in a positive turgidity- THE RECORD OF GROWTH-RESPONSE 423 <= variation, which produces an expansion of the responding zone of growth. The growth-response is thus the indirect effect of stimulation. : The rhythmic activity of internal tissue supplies its motive power to the zone of growth. This activity, depending on the tonic condition of the plant, constitutes the ‘inner stimuli’ to which growth is to be ascribed. CHAPTER XXXII THE EFFECTS ON GROWTH OF INTERNAL ENERGY AND EXTERNAL STIMULUS Characteristics common to growth and to other forms of rhythmic response : (1) Periodic groupings—(2) Effect of external stimulus in renewal of growth when at temporary standstill—(3) Renewal of growth-pulsation by positive turgidity-variation--(4) Effect of increased internal hydrostatic pressure— (5) Effect of ascent of sap on growth—Effect of temperature on growth —Comparison of various types of multiple response—Effect of external tension on growth—Effect of direct application of stimulus on the growing region —Similarities between motile and growth responses—Direct and indirect effects of stimulus, and laws of growth. HAVING shown in the last chapter that growth is a form of multiple or rhythmic response, I shall now proceed to demonstrate in detail the fact that in it alsoare found various phenomena which are characteristic of rhythmic response in general. (1) Periodic groupings.—-In the multiple response of Biophytum, and in the autonomous response of Desmodium, we have noticed the occurrence of various periodic groupings, the simplest of which consisted of an alternate waxing and waning of the pulses. In multiple growth-responses, similarly, we are able to detect such groupings, of which the simplest was shown in fig. 169 (c). When a continuous series of records is taken, extending over some time, these groupings undergo various changes, as fs illustrated in that figure, the three series (a), (6), and (c) having been taken with the same plant at different intervals. It will there be seen that the pulse-records in (c) represent an alternate waxing and waning of amplitude ; that in those of (4) the responses are small at the beginning and large at the end ; and, finally, that in the INTERNAL ENERGY AND EXTERNAL STIMULUS 425 responses of (a) this order is reversed, the amplitude being at first great, and undergoing a steady decrease to the end. (2) Effect of external stimulus on growth when at standstill.—It will be remembered that the rhythmic excita- tion of Desmodium comes to a standstill under unfavourable circumstances—that is to say, when the sum total of internal energy has fallen below par; and when this has happened, the application of fresh external stimulus is found to renew the activity. Growth-response, similarly, comes to a stop when the plant is in an unfavourable i condition with regard to light, tem- perature, or moisture. I shall now show that under such circumstances the application of external stimulus is found to be competent to renew growth. Taking a specimen of the hypocotyl of Zamarindus indicus, in which growth had come to standstill, I stimulated the plant by thermal means, and this was found to renew the multiple response of growth, as isclearly seeninfig.171. The pulses are here characterised by interesting periodic variations. The period of each is relatively long, the average FG. 171. Renewal of ; a : Growth-pulsation by Ther- value being about six minutes. In mal Stimulus in Zamarin- some other cases the renewed pulsa- ore a Grigaaye a tions were so rapid as almost to appear continuous. When the tonic condition of the plant was very low, the energy supplied by brief stimulation was only sufficient to maintain the rhythmic growth-activity for a short time, and after this the plant would again return to the state of standstill. It will thus be seen that when the tonic condition of the plant is below par, the applied external stimulus is absorbed, and, becoming latent, serves as internal energy for the production of growth-response (cf. p. 462). I shall adduce other instances of this in the course of the present chapter, 426 PLANT RESPONSE (3) Renewal of growth by positive turgidity-varia- tion.—We have seen that in Desmodzum in a state of standstill, increased internal hydrostatic pressure renewed the rhythmic Fic. 172. Initiation of Erectile Response in leaf by Supply of Water to partially Drought-rigored Mimosa activity. It was also stated in the last chapter, that growth is a responsive expression of the positive turgidity- variation. We have seen further that the mechanical expression of the positive turgidity-variation in a dorsi- ventral organ takes the form of erectile response. Thus this erectile response and growth-elongation are to be regarded as two different forms of expression of the same internal activity. If we take for example a plant of JZz7osa which is under- turgid, for want of sufficient supply of water, but not to the extent of drought-rigor, the leaves are found to assume a Fic. 173. Initiation of Growth- . pulsation by Small Supply of plant, under the same circum- Water to Drought-rigored Seed- ling of Cucurbita certain horizontal position, corre- sponding to the degree of tur- gidity. If we now supply the plant with water, poured on at the roots, the consequent sudden increase of suctional pumping activity is seen in the positiveerec- tile response of the leaf (fig. 172). Similarly, when an ordinary stances, has its growth brought The first thick dot represents toa standstill, the growth-elonga- application of water, which tion is found to be renewed on induces growth for three minutes ; F only. A second application at the application of a fresh supply the second dot renews it for the of water, This experiment was second time, carried out on a_ seedling of Cucurbita 12 cm. in height, growing in a small pot, which had come to growth-standstill for reasons described. Twoce. of water was supplied to the dry soil about the roots, and INTERNAL ENERGY AND EXTERNAL STIMULUS 427 growth-response was initiated after a latent period of eleven seconds (fig. 173). : It will be remembered that the positive turgidity-variation, on which growth depends, is hydrostatically transmitted at a much quicker rate than the state of excitation itself. In the present case, the responsive elongation in the growing region at a distance of about 12 cm. took place eleven seconds after the application of water at the root. There was a certain loss of time before the cells in the growing region became of sufficient turgidity to initiate growth. The small supply of water which had been given was enough to maintain growth for three minutes only, after which the plant came again toa standstill. Another 2 cc. of water was now applied, and the latent period was found reduced, as we should have expected, being now three seconds only. This was due to the fact that the cells had not now to absorb water before they could be sufficiently turgid. This renewed growth-activity was again, however, exhausted after about three minutes; and it was very interesting to observe how the response of growth followed, for a little while, after each such doling out of water. (4) Effect of increased internal hydrostatic pressure.— We found, in the case of Desmodium, when the cut petiole, carry- ing the motile leaflets, was subjected to increased hydrostatic pressure, applied by means of the U-tube, that the rhythmic activity of the leaflet,as shown by its quickening, was thereby increased (p. 320). Wesaw also in that case that this increased frequency, due to increased. pressure, reached an optimum, and that beyond this, under excessive pressure, the pulsations became irregular, or even came to a stop. In order, then, to study the effect of hydrostatic pressure on growth, I mounted the specimens—in this case an entire seedling of Balsam and a cut flower of Crzxum—in U-tubes supplied with water, and proceeded to take records, first under normal conditions—z.e. the level of water in the two limbs being the same—and then under a gradually increasing hydrostatic pressure. These records are made, it should be said, only when the rate of growth under the changed 428 conditions has become _ uniform. PLANT RESPONSE This occurs, generally speaking, after a period of variation which does not exceed two or three minutes. I give here a table embodying the results of the experiments on Ba/sam, and on Crinum, which show how increase of internal hydrostatic pressure increases the rate of growth up to an optimum, after which there is a diminution of growth. TABLE SHOWING EFFECT ON GROWTH OF INCREASED INTERNAL HYDROSTATIC PRESSURE Balsam seedling Crinum Lily Pressure Rate of growth Pressure Rate of growth Normal 5 cm. 10 cm. 15 cm. 20 cm. I | *0034 mm. per minute 9096 G75; Bs 0060 us 5 F 5s 2 "0095 9? > oe J ‘0130 ” ” »”» Normal 10 cm. 20 cm. 30 cm. 0038 mm. per minute *0044 33 ” > ‘O142 29 9 ” 0036 ” »” ” The curve seen in fig. 174 exhibits graphically the rela- tion between these internal pressures and corresponding growths in the case of Balsam seedling. It will be seen that after a certain moderate rate of growth has been attained by increase of pressure, the curve becomes a straight line ; that is to say, after this point, equal variation of pres- sure produces equal variation in the rate of growth. But in the first part of the curve, where the rate of growth is feeble, an equal increase of pressure causes a disproportionately large increase in the rate of growth. This is still more strikingly shown when the growth, to begin with, is zero— that is to say, at standstill; in such a case, by gradually increasing the internal pressure, we arrive at a point where growth begins abruptly, after which increasing pressure causes an increasing rate of growth. But if the pressure be now brought back to a point just below that at which growth was initiated, it is found not to be arrested, but to persist. Thus the curve does not here return upon itself. Since the various growth-curvatures are brought about by the variations of internal hydrostatic pressure caused by the INTERNAL ENERGY AND EXTERNAL STIMULUS 429 action of external stimulus, this quantitative demonstration of the effect of internal pressure on growth, is of much theoretical importance. I have already shown the connec- tio1 between mechanical response and growth-response, and demonstrated the fact that the erectile mechanical response and growth-elongation are but different expressions of increased internal activity. I shall now show how the suctional and growth re- sponses are related to each other, and in what manner the action of the former affects the latter. (5) Effect of ascent of sap on growth.— We have already seen that the positive turgidity-variation on which growth depends is brought about, under normal conditions, by the ascent of sap. As regards the ze Hae eC uve Showing Xelation between Internal latter, we have seen that when the Hydrostatic Pressure and root is subjected to the stimulating =< NE TN ie action of warm water there is a ; sudden augmentation induced in the rate of suction. The application of cold water, on the other hand, induces the converse effect. The mechanical response of the plant to warm or cold water, applied at the base, was shown to be manifested in the erection or depression of the leaves of Mimosa or Biophytum (p. 400). These agents are seen in the following experiments to produce parallel effects on growth. A growing Crinum Lily was taken, and its normal rate of growth ascertained to be ‘005 mm. per minute. Ice-cold water was now applied at its base, and this was found to cause an almost immediate arrest of growth. As the temperature, however, was gradually restored to that of the surroundings, the rate of growth was also slowly recovered. Five minutes after, the rate was only 430 PLANT RESPONSE ‘OOI mm. per minute, or one-fifth of the original rate. It was only after about half an hour that the original rate of growth was once more attained. I next applied warm water at the base, with the result that the rate of growth was almost instantaneously enhancedi to ‘125 mm. per minute, or twenty-five times the normal! That this effect was not due to the rise of temperature as such, is shown by the fact that it was almost instantaneous, and that, moreover, as will be shown in the next chapter, the maximum rate of growth of Crzmum at the optimum tem- perature is only about three or four times as great as the normal. . From these experiments we see that the energy applied at the root is transmitted hydraulically to the growing region by the ascent of sap, where a certain amount of work is per- formed in causing an increase of turgidity, and thus producing in the cells a state of tension. Growth is now caused not simply by the presence of water, but rather by the energy conveyed by that water. It is well to bear in mind, at this point, that the mobility or plasticity of the responding grow- ing region is also an important factor in the production of growth ; for if the molecular mobility of the zone of growth be in any way reduced, the transmitted pressure, which was formerly effective, will now become ineffective to bring about growth. (6) Effect of temperature on growth.—We have seen, in studying the pulsatory movements of Desmodium, that the rise of temperature, within-certain moderate limits, increased the rhythmic activity of the plant, as shown in the increased frequency of pulsation. Ata maximum temperature, again, above 40° C. these movements almost disappeared, there being now produced very rapid oscillations, of so small an amplitude as to be almost incapable of detection. This will be seen in fig. 175, where the normal pulsations, of a period of 2'5' at 30° C., are seen reduced to a period of only 10” at 42° C. With this, the amplitude also is so far reduced as to be visible only on very careful inspection. We meet with INTERNAL ENERGY AND EXTERNAL STIMULUS 431 corresponding phenomena in growth-response. It will be shown in the next chapter that the rate of growth increases with the rise of the temperature up to a certain optimum. At a determinate maximum, however, which is about 44° C., growth is arrested, but this arrest does not, as we have just seen in the corresponding instance of Desmodzum, imply a total cessation of internal activity. In making experiments on a seedling of Balsam, 1 obtained the record shown in the upper part of fig. 176, the temperature being 34° C., which is below the optimum. The average period of a single pulse Fic. 175. Photographic Record showing the Slow Pulsations of Large Amplitude of Desmodium Leaflet at 30° C. to become very much Quickened and Reduced in Amplitude at 42° C. was in this case 12'5 seconds, and owing to the considerable amplitude of each pulsation, combined with its incomplete recovery, the average of the resultant rate of growth was as much as ‘074 mm. per minute. On now raising the temperature to 44° C. I obtained the lower of the two records in fig. 176, showing no resultant growth. It is interesting to observe the process by which the cessation of growth comes about in this case. For it is clearly seen from the record that there is no cessation of activity. On the contrary, we find that the frequency of oscillation has become increased from four pulsations to ten, 432 PLANT RESPONSE in the course of 50’. The average period has thus fallen from 125’ at 34° C. to 5’ at 44° C. The amplitude of pul- sation at the same time is found to be decreased, and this, with the fact that recovery is now complete, accounts for the resultant cessation of growth. Comparison of vari- ous types of multiple response.—At this point, it is worth while to com- pare two or three types of multiple response. In Liophytum we have seen that, by reason of incom- plete recoveries from xega- tive turgidity - variation, the multiply-responding leaflet gradually becomes Fic. 176. Growth-pulsation seen in 2 Seedling of Balsam depressed below its ori- Upper record shows slow pulsations with ginal level. Incontrast to incomplete recoveries at 34° C. Lower is . record shows quickened pulsations with this we have in growth- complete recoveries at 44°C. The magnification employed is 500 times. respoMse those incomplete recoveries from fosztive turgidity-variations which have the effect of gradual elonga- tions In the pulsation of Desmodium again we have an intermediate instance where, response and recovery being: equal, the responding organ is ultimately neither raised nor — depressed. It is interesting to note, therefore, that in raising the temperature of a growing organ to the maximum, and thus abolishing the resultant elongation, we bring on a con- dition of equality of response and recovery which in so far resembles the pulsation of Desmodium. Effect of external tension on growth.—Great advan- tages are afforded by the method of magnified record, which enables us to detect instantly the immediate and after effects on the specimen of any changes of external conditions. It is INTERNAL ENERGY AND EXTERNAL STIMULUS) 433 thus easy to obtain exact records of the effect of tension on growth. The normal record is first taken, with the very slight tension exerted by the recording lever itself. The short arm of the lever, -5 cm. in length, is, it should be remembered, attached to the growing organ. A rider, half a gramme in weight, can be placed on the longer arm of the ievet.-at @istances of “5, I, 1°5, 2, 2°5, or. 3 cm. from the ful- crum. The effective tension may thus be gradually in- creased, and the corresponding effects on growth recorded. It may be stated here that, generally speaking, any sudden change of external conditions, such as sudden cooling, sudden warming, or sudden variation of tension, acts on the organ as an external stimulus; and I shall presently show that an external stimulus always induces a contraction or retardation of growth. When the organ is subjected to sudden increase of tension, the preliminary effect of con- traction occurs therefore, as we should expect. But after this temporary disturbance has disappeared we are able to observe the permanent effect of increase of tension on growth. For these experiments I took different specimens of Crznaum Lily, and the results obtained show that increase of tension enhances the rate of growth. This increase, however, appears to reach a limit at a certain optimum point, beyond which increase of tension would seem to retard growth. The follow- ing table exhibits the results of two experiments on different specimens of Crznum. TABLE SHOWING EFFECT OF TENSION ON RATE OF GROWTH Specimen A || SEceiMen B Tension Rate of growth | Tension é Rate of Je mm. | | mm. Weight of lever alone | ‘oo40 per min. | Weight of lever | *0036 per min. I gr. + lever “ONTO Meow es yet lear Geelncto ey sli) FOOT Ol ast) by oe ae POOn4G.' son ys nl) abana Mg | Wil sy pp Rpowicnect ~~ 9'5 "0045 5; ” I Beh 2h Se 6 55 | “O30 ,, ” | ZEN.) Peas TONG AOW We eric 2°5 gr. + . > “0050 ”? oe) 434 PLANT RESPONSE Effect of direct application of stimulus on the grow- ing region.—Many of the phenomena of growth-curvature are brought about, as we shall see, by means of the changes induced by external stimulus in the rate of growth, and there is much misconception as to whether the effect of stimulus is to enhance or to retard growth. I shall be able to show that this misconception is the result of the complexity of the problem, depending (1) on the tonic condition of the tissue, (2) on whether the stimulus is internal or external, and (3) on the point of application of stimulus. The fundamental effect of stimulus is, however, very definite. But at the beginning of our investigation we are met by a question of great importance, namely, as to whether the effect of stimulus on a growing, is in any way different from that on a stationary, organ. We have seen, for example, that a fully grown style of Datura, in which growth has come to a stop, Fic. 177. Photographic €Xhibits on the application of local ace of Responses of stimulus the usual contractile response Mature Style of Datura : alba to External Thermal (fig. 177). On the completion of re- anaes covery again, the organ returns to its original length. Hence the base-line of a series of these responses is horizontal. I shall now pass, by means of intermediate links, from this to the response of growing organs. And first I shall take the response of a style of Datura, in which, for want of a sufficient supply of internal energy, growth has come to a temporary stop. On applying thermal stimulus, at intervals of a minute, the first five responses are seen to be practically like those of the stationary style of Datura (fig. 178, cf. 177). But a portion of the stimulus applied is being absorbed and held latent in the organ, thus increasing the internal energy, or tonic condition. The result of this is seen in the renewal of growth at the sixth response. The stimulus now, therefore, finds bifurcated expression in maintaining response, and in renewing growth, as is seen by the trend downwards of the INTERNAL ENERGY AND EXTERNAL STIMULUS 435 hitherto horizontal base-line. This bifurcation causes the first contractile response of the now growing organ—that is the sixth response of the record-—to be smaller than usual. But as the tonic condition is established, and the molecular mobility of the responding organ is increased, the contractile response becomes larger, and growth goes on at a certain steady rate. From this intermediate link we pass on to the case of response to stimulus of a style of Datura which is in a state of uniform growth (fig. 179). Here also we find that stimulus produces the normal contractile effect. All these clearly demonstrate that the response of growing organs is in no way different from that of stationary organs. In a stationary organ, sti- mulation produces negative turgidity-variation, resulting in contraction. The same contraction is seen ingrowing Fic. 178. Photographic Record of : Responses of Style of Datera alba organs, causing a temporary in which Growth had come to a . srary S retardation of the rate of Tempo y oop h The up curve shows contraction. As growth. long as the base-line is horizontal, The retardation of srowth growth is seen moh Be at See ° ; : : Renewal of growth at sixth re- which is caused in a growing sponse, after which growth-elon- gation is shown by the trend of organ by external stimulus the base-line downwards. may be exhibited in a some- what different way. We may use the Method of Balanced Record, by which the normal rate of growth is made to appear as a neutral horizontal line. For the purpose of this experiment I took the growing peduncle of a Eucharis Lily. The balanced record (fig. 180) is here seen to be horizontal, save for minute autonomous oscillations about the neutral line. Sti- mulation, in this case, was produced by tetanic electric shocks from an induction coil, which were applied on the growing FEZ 436 PLANT RESPONSE organ, by means of non-polarisable electrodes, connected, one a little above, and the other a little below, the growing region. According to the conditions of the experiment, the balanced horizontal base-line will represent uniform growth under normal turgidity. An up curve will here represent a rate of growth below the normal, or a retardation ; a down curve, on the contrary, will represent a rate of growth above the normal, or an_ acceleration. When stimulus is applied a responsive negative turgidity- variation is induced, in conse- Fic. 179. Photographic Record of quence of which there is a Response of Growing Style temporary disturbance of the of Datura alba to External Sp lee balanced growth-record. The curve thus produced exhibits the effect of stimulus, and recovery from that effect. It will be observed from the figure that on the application of tetanic electric shocks for two seconds, a responsive retardation of Fic. 180. Balanced Record of Response in Growing Peduncle of Eucharis Lily to Electrical Stimulation Up curve here represents retardation of growth. First response is to stimulus of two seconds’, the second to stimulus of three seconds’, duration. growth was induced, as seen in the up curve, and that there was a recovery after an interval of nine minutes from the time of application of stimulus. It will also be noticed that during the process of recovery in this particular case, the rate INTERNAL ENERGY AND EXTERNAL STIMULUS 437 of growth is above the normal, which is due to the fact that the internal energy has been augmented by the absorption of the stimulus applied.'| A stimulus of longer duration, that is to say of three seconds, was next applied, and the responsive retardation had now a greater amplitude than before, the period of restoration being also longer, that is to say, sixteen minutes. Similarities between motile and growth responses.— We have, then, in growth-response an exact parallel to the mechanical responses given by pulvinated or anisotropic organs. The following tabular statement will show the reason of this fundamental parallelism between responses whose modes of indication are so widely different : TABULAR STATEMENT SHOWING COMPARATIVE EFFECTS OF STIMULUS IN PULVINATED AND GROWING ORGANS Mechanical response Growth response — ee = | ae Liffect of normal turgidity : | Effect of nor nel tur. eae Normal horizontal position of leaf. | Uniform rate of growth. Local action of external stimulus : Local act.on of external stimulus : Contraction ; Contraction ; Diminution of turgidity ; Diminution of turgidity ; and concomitant depression of leaf. and concomitant depression of rate of growth. Action of internal energy exhibited by | Action of internal energy exhibited by (a) Recovery : | (a) Recovery : Re-establishment of turgidity and | e-establishment of turgidity and | gradual return of leaf to normal | gradual return of organ to normal horizontal position. | rate of growth. (4) Increased hydrostatic pressure: | (4) Increased hydrostatic pressure : Erection of leaf. | Increased rate of growth. In studying the mechanical response of plants we found that direct application of stimulus to the responding organ always produced a response characterised by the negative turgidity-variation, that is to say, a depression of the leaf. We also saw that internal energy, inducing positive turgidity- variation, caused the opposite response, that is to say, an erection of the leaf ; and that an increase of the internal energy ' Other records showing the effect of external stimulus on growth will be found in Chapter XXXIV. 438 PLANT RESPONSE of the organ was, in some cases, brought about by-that in- creased suctional activity by which energy was transmitted, by means of water forced into the responding organ. This increased suctional activity was due in its turn to external stimulation of the roots. Or a similar transmission of energy to the responding organ might be the result of stimulation applied at a distance on the stem, in consequence of which a wave of positive turgidity-variation would travel towards the responding organ from the excited point. For the exhibition of this latter effect, however, it is necessary that the dzrect excitatory effect of stimulation should not be conducted to the organ. This condition is met if the point of stimulation be at a sufficient distance, or when the intervening tissue is not a good conductor of excitation. Again, the plant as a whole, it must be remembered, has its internal energy raised, as the after-effect of the absorption of stimulus from its sur- roundings. Direct and indirect effects of stimulus and laws of growth.-—In the phenomenon of growth-response we have a case which is exactly parallel. Direct stimulation of a growing organ always induces a negative turgidity-variation, with a concomitant responsive retardation of growth. Any- thing, on the other hand, which increases the internal energy, brings about the opposite effect, of positive turgidity-variation, with concomitant responsive increase in the rate of growth. This may be induced by a favourable rise of temperature, or by stimulating the root, and so increasing the ascent of sap. It may also be brought about by stimulating a distant point, and so causing a wave of positive turgidity-variation to be transmitted to the responding organ, the stimulated point being at a sufficient distance to prevent the direct effect of stimulus from reaching it. And, finally, the energy ab- sorbed from external stimulus may, as an after-effect, increase the internal energy of the plant. The increase of internal energy, under all these different conditions, we shall for the sake of convenience designate as the INDIRECT EFFECT OF STIMULUS. INTERNAL ENERGY AND EXTERNAL STIMULUS 439 The laws of growth are therefore : (1) The response of a growing organ is the same as that of a stationary organ. Direct application of stimulus, inducing con- traction, retards the rate of growth. (2) The effect of indirect stimulation is to increase the internal energy, and thus augment the rate of growth. SUMMARY The multiple response of growth is characterised by all the peculiarities seen, for instance, in the autonomous response of Desmodium. The rhythmic responses of growth exhibit periodic groupings. External stimulus is found to renew growth in organs in which, owing to the deficit of internal energy, it had come to a temporary standstill. The increase of internal hydrostatic pressure, up to an optimum, increases the rate of growth. The effect of increased internal hydrostatic pressure is exhibited in the case of the leaf of MWzmosa by an erectile response. In a growing organ the effect of increased turgidity is shown by growth-elongation. A drought-rigored d/zmosa on being supplied with water responds by erection of the leaf; and similarly, a plant in which growth, owing to drought- rigor, has come to a standstill, responds, on being supplied with water, by renewed growth-elongation. When the root of Mimosa is supplied with ice-cold water, the responding leaf, owing to the consequent arrest of ascent of sap, becomes depressed. The increased suctional activity, again, which is caused by a supply of warm water to the root, induces an erectile response of the leaf. Similarly, corresponding varia- tions in the rate of ascent of sap, brought about in a growing : plant by the application of cold and warm water to the roots, cause respective depressions and accelerations of the rate of growth. The energy supplied at the roots is hydraulically trans- 440 PLANT RESPONSE mitted to the growing region, and finds expression in the work of growth. A rise of temperature, up to the optimum, enhances the rate of growth. Ata maximum temperature of about 44° C. growth is apparently arrested. This is not, however, due to any rigor or arrest of internal activity, but to the fact that in each pulsation of growth the constituent response and recovery are now equal. There is thus no resultant growth-elongation. At such a temperature the amplitude of pulsation is re- duced, and the frequency increased, as in Desmodium. Longitudinal tension has the effect, up to an optimum, of increasing the rate of growth. The effect of external stimulus on a growing, is precisely the same as on a stationary, organ, that is to say, a respon- sive contraction. On account of this contraction, and con- comitant negative turgidity-variation, growth is retarded, as the direct effect of the action of external stimulus, External stimulus, however, when absorbed and held latent by the tissue, has the effect of increasing the internal energy of the plant. This indirect effect of stimulus causes acceleration of growth. CHAPTER XXXIII ON THE RELATION BETWEEN TEMPERATURE AND GROWTH, - AND THE ACCURATE DETERMINATION OF OPTIMUM AND MAXIMUM POINTS General consideration of difficulties of accurate determination of effects of temperature on growth—Four accurate methods: (1) Method of discon- tinuous observations—Accurate regulation of temperature by electrolytic rheostat —(2) Method of continuous observations—Thermo-crescent curve— Determination of the optimum point —(3) Method of balance—(4) Method of excitatory response—Translocation of the optimum point. DETERMINATIONS of the effect of temperature on growth are usually carried out either by observing a single plant for several hours in succession, or by obtaining the average growth of various groups of plants, kept under different temperatures. In the latter case the individual peculiarities of different specimens cause them to give values which are more or less discrepant, but this fact is to some extent neutralised by taking the average of a large number. These methods, however, are all very laborious, and the results not highly consistent. Whichever method be adopted, we have to remember that in all cases in which observations stretching over periods of several hours are required, the growth of the plant will be liable to spontaneous variations, resulting from the periodicities impressed upon it by the changing conditions of its environment. It is, perhaps, owing to this fact that the results obtained by various authorities have been so widely divergent. For example, the optimum temperature for growth of Zea mazs was found by Sachs to be 34° C. and by Koppen to be 302° C. Clearly, the perfect method would be one in which we should be able to measure the effects of temperature alone on 442 PLANT RESPONSE the growth of a particular plant, so that the varying factors of age and constitution, or tonic condition, should remain constant. And, further, it should be possible to carry out this determination of growth at different temperatures in a time so short that the spontaneous variations of the plant, if any, would be insignificant. Only by such a method could we hope to obtain results which would be reliable and consistent. Keeping these considerations in view, I have been fortunate enough to be able to devise four distinct methods of determin- ing the effect of temperature on the rate of growth, the perfection of which may be gauged from the fact that the results of all agree with and corroborate each other within a fraction of a degree. With some of these, the entire experi- ment on the different rates of growth at various temperatures, ranging from ordinary through optimum to maximum, can be carried out within about half an hour. The determination of any single cardinal point, such as the optimum, can always again be made within five minutes. The investigation thus gains by simplicity and experimental accuracy, and observa- tions may be made on many different specimens within a very short period. I shall now proceed to describe these different methods. (1) The method of discontinuous observations.—We shall first take the method by which rates of growth are recorded at different temperatures, say one degree apart. Some means of raising the temperature to exactly the required point, and maintaining it unchanged during the time of experiment, is an essential condition of all these investigations. This I have been able to accomplish in the following way. ‘The electrical heating coil inside the plant chamber is put in connection with an external battery. The heat given out, and the consequent permanent rise of temperature in the chamber, depend on the intensity of the current that flows through the heating coil. This current again may be progressively regulated by the interposition of an electrolytic rheostat in the circuit, the resistance of which RELATION BETWEEN TEMPERATURE AND GROWTH 443 can be subjected to gradual variation. The electrolytic rheostat consists of two semicircular troughs (fig. 181). By turning the handle, say to the right, the electrolytic resistance interposed is continuously increased, diminishing the current, and hence diminishing the temperature inside the chamber. Rotation of the handle in the opposite direction produces the opposite effect, that is to say, raises the temperature inside the chamber. Thus, by proper manipulation of the handle of the rheostat, the chamber can be raised to any temperature, Fic. 181. Semicircular Electrolytic Rheostat interposed in Heating Coil Current enters the first trough, filled with zinc sulphate solution, by the electrode, z', and is led to the second trough by the diagonal metallic connector, Dp’. By turning the index-arm, I, clockwise, the interposed resistance is increased, and the heating current thus diminished. Rota- tion in the opposite direction diminishes the resistance and increases the heating current. which can then be maintained uniform for any length of time, by keeping the rheostatic resistance constant. From a previous experiment, the temperature-values of the position of the index in connection with the handle can be ascertained and marked. Thus by turning the handle to any given index number, say of 31° C.,the temperature of the chamber will be found to be raised permanently to that value. A delicate thermometer graduated in twentieths of degrees is placed in the chamber, and affords an independent indication of the 444 PLANT RESPONSE temperature thus attained. It is also necessary, for reasons to be fully explained in a subsequent chapter, that the specimen should not receive thermal radiation from the heating coil, as such radiation, I find, retards growth. A shield of mica, opaque to thermal radiation, is interposed between the heating coil and the plant, which is thus subjected only to the action of changes of temperature. For many of my experiments I selected specimens of the Crzzum Lily, on account of its extreme regularity of growth, which is so uniform that on adjusting the record under balance, the external conditions being constant, the line of record remained horizontal for a period of certainly over an hour. The rate of growth at the temperature of the room, say 30° C,, is first taken on the recording drum, which is covered with paper divided into millimetres. The horizontal distance or abscissa represents time, which, with the particular speed of drum which I used was 6 mm. per minute. The ordinate represents growth-elongation, and as the growth-recorder, or crescograph, produces a magnification of 1,000 times, I mm. distance of the ordinate is equal to an actual growth of ‘ool mm. The ordinate corresponding to an abscissa of 6 mm. would thus be equivalent to a growth in thousandths of a millimetre per minute. The rheostat handle is now turned to the index-number corresponding to the raising of the temperature of the chamber by 1°C. There is first a variable period of rise of temperature, after which a permanent degree is attained. During this preliminary stage, variation of temperature acts as a stimulus, giving rise to responsive contraction or retardation of growth. But after this transient disturbance, the growth attains a constant rate, characteristic of the given temperature. These peculiarities will be better understood on following the record given in fig. 182—reduced here to half the original size— which was taken with a specimen of Crinum Lily, during ten minutes. The record during the first five minutes is for the temperature of 34° C. It will be seen that in two minutes the growth-elongation is fifteen divisions, and as each division RELATION BETWEEN TEMPERATURE AND GROWTH 445 represents ‘ool mm., the rate of growth is thus ‘0075 mm. per minute. The rheostatic handle was afterwards turned to the mark 35° C. With the particular battery power used in this case, the permanent rate of rise to 35° C. was attained after a period of three minutes. It will be seen from the record that the stimulus of sudden variation of temperature caused a contractile twitch, after which , growth proceeded at a very . rapid rate during the variable period. But as soon as the temperature of the chamber had attained a permanent condition —1.e. 35° C.—the rate of growth became constant. The attain- ment of this constant rate was practically simultaneous with the attainment of the permanent temperature condition. The lag, in any case, if it existed, could not be more than fifteen seconds. One curious and interesting : Fic. 182, Record of Growth in fact to be fully explained later, Crinum at Temperature of 34° C. . ° . 9c° which was noticed in the course and 35° C. of the experiments, was that The dotted line represents the variable period of temperature the amount of contractile twitch change. Note the contractile aie 5 = twitch and transient highly ac- went on increasing during the celerated growth which follows. variable period, as the tempera- The rate of growth became con- - f Ak stant when the temperature be- ture was raised each time I° C., came permanent at 35° C. mendieso to. 135° C., but. after this point practically disappeared. The permanent rate of growth, then, at a temperature of 35° C., is, as will be seen from the figure, twenty-four divisions per two minutes, or ‘O12 mm. per minute. In this way, by taking successive records at different temperatures, I obtained the following rates of growth, in the cases of Crzzum Lily and the peduncle of Crocus. 446 PLANT RESPONSE Crinum Lily | Peduncle of Crocus Temperature Growth in mm. per minute | Temperature Growth in mm. per minute | ey Or "0036 | 204. "0059 RISC. “0040 Beate: *O100 rt ie “0050 35° C. ‘O145 BRciIC: 0055 SOc. "0062 3426. 0075 38° CG. +0030 35° C. ‘0085 40° C. 0010 362... "0050 a7 "0035 | (2) Method of continuous observations.—The method which I have just described gives us results which, though obtained at closely consecutive temperatures, are nevertheless discontinuous. There is, besides, some loss of time involved during the variable period, in addition to which there is the factor of transient stimulation during sudden changes of temperature. For these reasons I was anxious to perfect some method by which the curve of growth should afford a continuous means of obtaining the rate of growth at all temperatures. I also wished to eliminate from this record — the preliminary disturbance caused by sudden change of temperature. I was enabled to do this in practice by bringing about a sradual and continuous rise of temperature, instead of the former sudden variations by steps. This was effected by turning the handle of the electrolytic rheostat at a rate so graduated that the rise of temperature within the chamber was uniform. As it was necessary to complete the experiment within not too long a period, I found that a rise of 1° C. per 2'5 minutes was sufficient to meet the requirements of the case. This means a rise of 1° C. in fifteen seconds. An observer watches a delicate thermometer, which is placed in the plant chamber, with his hand on the handle of the rheostat. By means of this, and a chronometer beating seconds, he is able to regulate the uniform rise of temperature with the greatest nicety. Should the rate be too quick, it may be reduced by the slightest turn of the handle towards the increase of Ss .-hCCl RELATION BETWEEN TEMPERATURE AND GROWTH 447 resistance, or vzce versa. After a little practice the process of regulation becomes almost instinctive. The record of growth is now taken continuously on the revolving drum, and the thermo- crescent curve obtained under these conditions of continuous variation of temperature is seen to be extremely regular, giving data by which we may determine the rate of growth at any point of the curve (fig. 183). The revolving drum gave, as already said, a movement of the recording surface of 6 mm, per 30° 31° 32° 33° 34°35" 36° 37° 38° 39° 40" 4 47 43°44" pasa Fic, 183. Thermo-crescent Curve of Growth in Crinum Lily under Continuously Increasing]Temperature minute. That length of the abscissa would therefore repre- sent one minute of time; and since the rise of temperature was regulated, at 1° C. of rise per 2°5 minutes, intervals of 15 mm. in the abscissa also represent 1° C. in temperature. With the magnification used, 1 mm. of the ordinate represents a growth-elongation of a thousandth part of a mm. In order, therefore, to obtain from this curve the rate of growth at any given temperature, say at 34° C., we have to find the elonga- 448 PLANT RESPONSE tion per minute at that point. The rise of temperature in one minute, then, under the experimental conditions described, is through ‘4° C. The growth-elongation of the specimen, therefore, while the temperature is rising from 33°8° C. to 342° C., gives us the rate of growth for the mean temperature of 34° C. This is found in the magnified record to be 10 mm. The absolute value of the rate of growth is thus ‘Ol mm. per minute. In this way we can determine from the curve the rate of growth corresponding to any temperature. It will thus be seen how in the course of an experiment Jasting for thirty-five minutes only, we are able to obtain data which give us the various rates of growth through a wide range of temperatures. This operation can, moreover, be made entirely automatic. — The breadth of the circular electrolytic trough may be appro- priately varied at different parts of the circle, so that turning the handle through equal arcs raises the temperature of the plant chamber by equal degrees. The handle of the rheostat may then be rotated by the recording drum itself. Hence in the record, equal lengths of the abscissa will represent not only equal times, but also equal rises of temperature. And finally by taking the record photographically, the whole pro- cess becomes automatic. From the data furnished by fig. 184 we obtain the following table: TABLE SHOWING RATES OF GROWTH AT DIFFERENT TEMPERATURES IN FLOWER OF CRINUM LILY. Temperature | Growth in mm. per minute Temperature Growth in mm. per minute eho (Cr “0040 a{a~ (Ce 0070 Spal OP 0057 Be G. 0045 g2>C. "0075 38° C. 0030 53° ‘0087 og OF ‘0020 34° C. "0100 Hole G ‘OO12 Z5°C, ‘O110 AZcas: 0005 Cee | ‘OII3 Agra, “0002 The curve shown in fig. 184 exhibits the relation between these various temperatures and their corresponding rates of growth. It is here seen that the rate gradually rises till we RELATION BETWEEN TEMPERATURE AND GROWTH 449 approach the optimum point, which in the present case is 35°5° C. After this there is a steep fall, and growth is almost abolished at a maximum temperature at or near 45°C. This arrest of growth does not mean arrest of internal activity. I have in the last chapter (fig. 176) explained why, in spite of the persistence of internal activity, there is in this case no resultant growth. It is to be borne in mind that as the curve of growth in fig. 183 is continuous, the rate of growth at Fic. 184. Curve showing Relation between Temperature and Rate of Growth, as deduced from the Thermo-crescent Curve in fig. 181 points intermediate to those specified may be determined from it. From a large number of experiments which I have carried © out on Crinum Lily, I find that the optimum temperature is very constant, not varying by as much as one-tenth of a degree from the mean value of 35°5° C. as the optimum tem- perature. In the first portion of the curve, as the temperature rises from 30° C. to 35°5° C. the rate of growth is seen to increase, from ‘004 to ‘O1125 mm. per minute, or to nearly three times its first value. The fall, beyond the optimum, is ° C. the rate of growth has GG steeper than this rise. At 37 A50 PLANT RESPONSE fallen to 0045 mm. per minute, or almost the same as at 30° C. Thus, while 5°5° C. of rise of temperature before the optimum enhanced the rate of growth by three times, a further rise of only 1°5° C. beyond that point was sufficient to bring it back to almost the same value as at 30°C. The individual characteristics of each specimen are seen, not by any perceptible variation of the optimum point, but rather by differences in the steepness, during rise or fall, of the curve. With some specimens, for example, the increase of rate of growth during an equal rise of temperature from 30° C. to 35°5° C. is only half of that seen in the figure. The steepness of fall, on the other hand, beyond the optimum may be much greater; that is to say, arise of 1° C. or less above the optimum will sometimes reduce the rate of growth to its value at 30° C. (3) The Method of Balance.—I shall now describe an extremely delicate method of determining the rate of growth at different temperatures, which is especially suited for the exact determination of the optimum point. A balanced line of record is first obtained by the turning of the balancing wheel of the Crescograph (fig. 168). This regulates the difference of level of the syphon tube, until the spot of light is stationary at the temperature of the room. As the tem- perature is now raised and the rate of growth increased, the balancing wheel has to be rotated, say to the right, in order to keep the spot of light stationary. The reading of the circular scale at different temperatures thus gives the balanced readings for the corresponding rates of growth at those temperatures. A previous calibration of the value of the circular scale enables us to determine the absolute growth- movements at various temperatures. For the determina- tion of the optimum point, however, this is not necessary. All that has to be done in this case is to keep the spot, which would otherwise drift to the right under a constantly increasing rate of growth, on the point of balance, by the right-handed rotation of the balancing wheel. This must be done as long as the temperature and rate of growth are RELATION BETWEEN TEMPERATURE AND GROWTH 451 ascending towards the optimum. On reaching and passing that point, however, it is found that the spot of light, which has hitherto tended to move to the right, now has its move- ment suddenly reversed to the left, thus necessitating a corresponding reversal of the balancing rotation. This turning point is extremely sharp and well defined, and enables us to make an accurate determination of the optimum tem- perature within less than a tenth of a degree. From a previous knowledge that the optimum point lies, say, between 35° C. and 36° C., the rise of temperature from 35° C. to 36° C. within the chamber may be adjusted to take place in five minutes, that is to say a rise of one-twentieth of a degree per fifteen seconds. The second observer, watching the delicate thermometer in the plant chamber, calls out at every twentieth of a degree of rise of temperature. The first observer, at the recording drum, notes the temperature of the turning point. It has been said before that the permanent rate of growth for any given temperature is always established in less than fifteen seconds of reaching it. The possible error, owing to this lag, could not therefore exceed one-twentieth of a degree. TABLE SHOWING CIRCULAR READINGS OF BALANCING WHEEL AT DIFFERENT TEMPERATURES, | Crinum Lily ine Hypocotyl of Balsam Temperature | Circular reading Temperature | Circular reading | | 302°C. 0° 30" Cc: 0° | seo. 70° 0° G. | 12° Peek ee 171° | Cu Oe 42° 33° C. 270° | 33°C: 86° | BA iC. 405° Eye Ln 146° 35° C. 538° | 34°6° C. 170° 35°4° C. | 600° | Shae ni2° Boa. 494° S6.G 85° 37° C. | 354° 37° C. Sa | 38° C. 140° 38° C. 52 | The turning point of another specimen was || The turning point of another specimen was | found to be 35°5° C. found to be 34°4° C. I give above two sets of readings of the balancing wheel, made during two experiments for the determination of the GG2 A452 PLANT RESPONSE optimum point for Crzzum Lily and the hypocotyl ot Balsam The balanced reading at 30° C. is taken as zero. The ad- justment of the stop-cocks for regulation of outflow was different in the two cases. (4) The method of excitatory response.—The method which I am about to describe—and by which the relative rates of growth at different temperatures are afforded in- directly—is one of much theoretical importance, for it proves what I have already suggested, that growth is a phenomenon of excitatory response. This being so, it would follow that the reason why growth is at its optimum at about 35° C. in the case of most tropical plants, is that the excitability of the tissue is greatest at that temperature. The different excitabilities at different temperatures might further be expected, this being true, to offer an independent indication of the characteristic rate of growth of the tissue at those temperatures. The excitability of the tissue can be tested, in the case of radial organs, by its longtitudinal contractile response to external stimulus, which, as we have seen, will be represented in growing organs by a retardation of growth, proportionate to the excitability. We must bear in mind, at this point, certain differences between responsive effects in mature and in growing organs. In the former, owing to the increase of internal energy brought about by rise of temperature, the tissue becomes over-turgid and the internal hydrostatic pressure is greatly increased. The contractile action of external stimulus is thus strongly resisted by the tissue, which in this way antagonises the normal extent of response (p. 338). Similarly, a closed india-rubber ball, fully distended with water, will not yield to any great extent when struck. But if we have, instead, a tube through which water is running, the flexible pipe when struck will yield, and cause a proportionate retardation or reversal of current behind. We have a case somewhat analogous in a growing organ. For here the tissue cannot be regarded as closed, since it is constantly elongating. It therefore represents, not a static RELATION BETWEEN TEMPERATURE AND GROWTH 453 condition of rest, but a dynamic condition of equilibrium, and it will offer little effective resistance to excitatory con- traction. We shall therefore expect that in growing organs similar stimuli will induce responsive effects varying in pro- portion to the changes of excitability in the tissue, under different conditions. We saw, in the case of Crinum Lily, that the optimum temperature was near 35° C., and that at this optimum the rate of growth was something like one and a half to three times as great as at 30° C. At 37°C. we saw, further, that the rate of growth was again reduced, and had become equal to, or lower than, that at 30°C. We might therefore expect that, on recording the retardation of growth in response to external stimulus at three definite temperatures, say 30° C., 35° C., and 37° C., we should find it to be greatest at 35° C. being in fact at that point about one and a half to three times as great as at 30°C. The response at 37° C., on the other hand, which is beyond the optimum, would be much less than at 35° C., being equal to, or even less than, that at ‘saris; TABLE SHOWING VARIATION OF EXCITATORY MECHANICAL RESPONSE AT DIFFERENT TEMPERATURES Seite Response at Response at Response at temperature of 30° C. | temperature of 35° C. | temperature of 37° C. 2 wee ache ae vee = Electrical I. 10°5 divisions 37 divisions | 10°5 divisions | ) | fe i, 22 he 31 5 iis ajectoae | “ Sine" Senn sees s ‘ | Ghemablll. | 32 3 46 i 24 e | | > > on IV. 18 3 28 zs LOS) Sai .5 I have made numerous experiments completely bearing out these conclusions. The mode of experimental proce- dure is as follows: a balanced record is taken at the given temperature, and the growing organ is then subjected to a definite intensity of stimulation, which may consist of tetanic thermal or electrical shocks, lasting for twenty seconds. Records are then made of the resulting contractile retarda- A54 PLANT RESPONSE tion of growth at the different required temperatures. Three responses were taken at each temperature, and were found to be practically the same. Some of these records will be given in the next chapter (fig. 185). I have given the results of four such experiments, carried out on different specimens. The translocation of the optimum point.—We have thus seen how constant is the optimum point in the same species, under normal conditions; but, since we found that the otherwise constant death-point was liable to be shifted under the disturbance caused by the sudden variation of external conditions (p. 172), so it would appear probable that the optimum point also would be liable to transposition under the influence of similarly disturbing causes. The optimum point of the Crzxum Lily has been seen to lie, normally speaking, between 35'4° C. and 35'°5° C. After a night of heavy rain and gale, however, I found that the optimum point of a specimen of this Lily had fallen to 34°6° C. Under the action of a poison like copper sulphate, again, admi- nistered in such dilution as not to kill, but only to retard growth, I have observed the optimum point to be lowered to 34°5° C. In the case of dilute solution of sugar, however, which induces—as we shall see in the next chapter—an increase of growth-activity, I have found the optimum point to be raised to 36°6° C. Thus under normal conditions the optimum temperature for each species is extremely definite. But circumstances which increase or decrease the rate of growth abnormally, operate also to transpose the optimum point, in the same manner as the death-point was found to be translocated by external influences. SUMMARY The difficulties usually encountered in the accurate deter- mination of the effect of temperature on growth have been successfully overcome in the case of four distinct methods. In taking records of growth at different temperatures, it RELATION BETWEEN TEMPERATURE AND GROWTH 455 is found that the variation from one to another acts as a stimulus, and induces a transient retardation of growth. But this cause of disturbance is eliminated when the rise of temperature is made gradual and continuous. In this way, by taking a continuous record of growth under uniform rise of temperature, a thermo-crescent curve is obtained, that gives data from which the absolute values of growth at all temperatures may be obtained. From this curve we are also able to obtain an accurate determination of the optimum and maximum points. The Method of Balance also affords us, by means of a sharply defined turning point, an exact indication of the optimum point. The optimum point is very definite, and under normal conditions is always constant for a given species; but just as the death-point was found liable to be shifted under abnormal external conditions, so the optimum point also is apt to be transposed under similarly disturbing causes. That growth is a phenomenon of excitatory response is demonstrated by the fact that the growth-rate is increased or decreased at different temperatures, in proportion to the excitability of the tissue at the same points, as indicated by its contractile response. CHAPTER XXXIV ON AN ATTEMPT TO DETECT AND ‘MEASURE LATENT STIMULUS, AND ON THE STUDY OF PERIODIC AFTER- EFFECTS , Positive and negative after-effects—Extreme delicacy of the Method of Balance — Detection of absorbed stimulus by negative after-effect—Constancy of sum of direct and indirect after-effects—Latent component almost vanishing above the . optimum—Variation of receptivity--Direct and indirect response of plant in sub-tonic condition—Table showing direct and indirect effects at different temperatures—Is the change induced by stimulus always of an explosive chemical character ?—Relation between stimulus and response in different tonic conditions— After-effect —Factors which determine periodic after-effects : (1) Stimulus of light—(z) Temperature—(3) Chemical stimulus—(4) Tur- gidity—Continuous photographic record of the pulsations of Desmodiam— Record of periodic variation of rate of growth—Continuous photographic record of periodic variations of transpiration—Continuous photographic record of the variation of the rate of growth—Annual rings and seasonal periodicity. By regarding the plant as a machine, as we did in the course of the earlier chapters, we were enabled to understand the possi- bility of its absorbing, and holding latent, more or less of the incident stimulus (p. 124). The experimental demonstration of this would, however, be difficult, in the case of the ordinary response of motile organs ; for though we have seen that ex- ternal stimulus and the absorbed internal energy are opposite in their responsive effects, yet in the ordinary records of mechanical response it is not easy to discriminate that part of the effect which is due to the latter element; for while it is true that the presence of internal energy would tend to hasten the recovery, it is still impossible to distinguish with certainty a recovery so hastened from one which is natural. The fact that excess of stimulus is transformed into latent energy is demonstrated, however, by the occurrence of multiple response. —— DETECTION OF LATENT STIMULUS 457 Positive and negative after-effects.—We have seen that when a moderately strong stimulus acts ona responding organ, a short time elapses before the initiation of the re- sponse, and this is known as the latent period ; but when response has been initiated, it persists for some time, even on the cessation of the stimulus, and this is known as the after- effect. I shall, however, for important reasons, which will appear later, further distinguish it as the fosztcve after-effect. By the term positive after-effect, then, is meant the continua- - tion of a response evoked by external stimulus, on the cessation of that stimulus. We may, for example, imagine a heavy elastic spring immersed in a viscous fluid. If this be sub- jected to a sudden compressional blow, then, after a short latent period, it will begin to undergo compression, and this compressional movement will continue for some time, even on the cessation of the blow that caused it, thus exhibiting a positive after-effect. But a spring compressed in this manner contains some amount of latent or potential energy, on account of which it next begins to expand, exhibiting a movement opposite to the first. This second movement, due to the latent energy, we may distinguish as the negatzve after- effect. This negative after-effect, it should further be stated, may sometimes be separated from the direct effect by a con- siderable interval of time. This may be seen in a viscous wire subjected to a torsional impulse. After the twisting has ceased, some time elapses before the wire is seen to begin the contrary, or negative, movement of untwisting, which is accomplished very slowly, and may even take hours to complete. Now, it occurred to me that, in the response of growth, it was possible to find a means of detecting whether the external stimulus in the case of living tissue might, or might not, become partially latent, to be similarly manifested later, in the form of the negative after-effect ; for if we take a balanced horizontal record of growth, then the direct effect of external stimulus will be seen in that retardation which is shown in the shifting of the line—here, for convenience of 458 PLANT RESPONSE inspection, represented upwards (fig. 185). On the cessation of external stimulus, if the recovery of the excited region be merely passive, it is evident that this ascending line will gradually return to the horizontal, as in the third record of fig. 185; that is to say, the retarded will be exchanged by degrees for the normal rate of growth. But if some portion of the external stimulus be held latent in the tissue, this will go to increase the internal energy of the plant. Now, we have already seen that the effect of augmented internal energy is exhibited in an increase of the rate of growth above the normal, shown in a balanced response-curve by an opposite movement to that of retardation, constituting the negative after-effect. Such a negative after-effect, consisting of an enhanced rate of growth, will persist until the energy thus held latent is exhausted, when the curve will again return to the horizontal. Thus, the up curve will represent the direct effect of external stimulus, and the down curve the acceleration of growth due to absorbed stimulus, or the nega- tive after-effect. ‘Extreme delicacy of the Method of Balance.—Such transient variations in the rate of growth, occurring as the expression of the absorbed fraction of incident stimulus, would have been incapable of detection by the ordinary auxonometric method of growth-record ; for here, owing to the relatively slight magnification which is possible, it takes nearly half an hour to obtain data from which the normal rate of growth may be inferred. Another half-hour’s obser- vation would be necessary before we could infer the occur- rence of variation under changed conditions, and it is clear that, during a period relatively so long, the plant may undergo spontaneous changes. The after-effects, however, which we now wish to detect, are found to take place imme- diately, and to last for a few minutes only, in the case of moderate stimulation. Even with our crescographic arrange- ment, though the usual magnification is a thousand times, the variation constituting the after-effect is seen only in a slight change of the slope of the curve; but when the Method of i DETECTION OF LATENT STIMULUS A59 Balance is employed, the ordinary magnification is enough to show, in a very marked manner, all the phases of these transient variations. The records here reproduced have been in fact reduced to one-third of the originals. The sensitiveness of the arrangement can be very much exalted by observing the balanced line of light with its devia- tions, through a telescope placed at a distance. In this way, I have been able to detect a variation from the normal rate of growth, of so little asa two millionth part of I mm. per second, within so short a period of observation as ten seconds. Detection of absorbed stimulus by negative after- effect.— We must now revert to the question of the detection of latent stimulus by an increase in the rate of growth, and we shall first take that simple case in which there is no loss of energy from irreversible effects due to molecular friction. The energy of external stimulus will here find complete expression in doing external and internal work. If the external stimulus remain constant, the sum of these two —that is to say, the direct or immediate, and the indirect effects—will also remain constant ; but if, under the same circumstances, one of these factors, say the direct effect, should for any reason be enhanced, we might then expect that its complement, the indirect effect, would undergo a corresponding diminution ; while, if the direct effect should be small, the indirect effect would show augmentation. These theoretical considerations are found very strikingly verified in the experiment which I shall now describe. | first took a balanced record of growth in a specimen of Crinum Lily, at 30° C. This was then subjected to thermal shocks for five seconds. The direct response, as will be seen from the first record in fig. 185, which is reduced to one-third of the original, was a retardation of growth represented by 33 divisions. On the cessation of stimulus, however, the rate of growth did not at once return to the normal, but exhibited the effect of absorbed energy by an acceleration shown in the down curve, and represented by 13 divisions, after which it became normal. The same stimulus was now repeated, and 460 PLANT RESPONSE the direct effect was a retardation of 31, which was followed by an augmentation of 14 divisions. Thus the sum of these two effects is practically constant, being in one case 46, and in the other 45, divisions. Constancy of sum of direct effect and indirect after- effect.— This constancy, however, becomes still more remark- able when the same plant is raised to a temperature of 35° C. and subjected once more to the same stimulation. The direct effect is now shown by a retardation which may be repre- sented as 39, and the in- direct by 9, divisions. In the second response of this second series, we have a direct effect of 37, and an indirect effect of 8, divi- sions. Thus the sum of the first direct and indirect effects is 48, and the sum Fic. 185. Series of Responses of Growing of the second 45, divisions, iif contldtasfutcebifeae, the mean of the two at Temperatures 35°C. being 46°5 divisions, On comparing these records it will be seen while the mean at 30°C: that the direct efect incteases WP fo was 45's divisional effect of accelerated growth decreases. have found, then, not only Jey, ; at 27°C . is n0 latent component, 2 shown by that the sum of direct and lisse direct effect to normal jndirect effects at a given . temperature is practically constant when stimulus is the same, but also that this sum itself remains approximately constant at different tempera- tures within the optimum ; and, further, we see that as the excitability is increased in approaching the optimum, the direct effect also increases at the expense of the indirect. In other words, when the tissue is at its optimum tonic condition, its capacity for the absorption of stimulus being already fully satisfied, the external stimulus tends to be immediately expended, in direct response, allowing relatively little to become latent. DETECTION OF LATENT STIMULUS 461 Latent component almost vanishing above the opti- mum.—As an extreme instance of this, we may take the response at a temperature beyond the optimum, say at 37° C. Here, by its environmental conditions, the plant is already supplied with an excess of energy. And besides this, there is the fact which we have already noticed, that its general excitability is diminished, so as to be equal to, or less than, that at 30°C. At these two temperatures, then—of 30° C. and 37° C.—we have two conditions of excitability more or less the same, but with a different history. Below the optimum there is an unsatisfied capacity for absorption of stimulus, whereas above it this capacity has been fully met. It would therefore appear that the power of the tissue to hold stimulus latent is diminished progressively up to the optimum, till beyond that point it practically disappears. I obtained a remarkable confirmation of this inference in the course of my experiments. In the experi- ment just described, for example, the direct response was a retardation of twenty-four divisions, and there was no indirect effect, showing that little or no stimulus had become latent (fig. 185). Variation of receptivity.—In the experiments described, the sum of the direct and the indirect effects, up to the optimum, had been found to be approximately constant, that is to say, a total response of about forty-six divisions. At 37° C., how- ever, we see the total response reduced to twenty-four divisions without any Jatent component. And since the total response measures for us the amount of stimulus taken up by the tissue, it would appear that at 37° C. not only is the power to hold stimulus latent lost, but also that the general . receptivity of the tissue is very much reduced. It is thus seen that the condition of a tissue modifies its receptive power ; hence it is possible for different parts of the same organ—say, for instance, the tip and the growing region— being in different conditions, to possess different receptivities. Direct and indirect response of plant in sub-tonic condition.— Turning from this case of excess of energy to 462 PLANT RESPONSE that in which it is below par, the plant being in a sub-tonic condition of arrested growth, we find that external stimulus gives rise to little expression in direct response. External stimulus is found under such circumstances mainly to increase the store of internal energy, in consequence of which we obtain the indirect response of renewed growth. In such a condition, the plant has a great capacity for the absorption of external energy. After growth has commenced, the energy of incident stimulus finds bifurcated expression, by inducing direct or immediate response, and by the indirect or negative after-effect, of enhanced rate of growth (p. 434). The sum total of these two—external stimulus being the same— remains approximately constant, till the optimum tonic con- dition is reached. The direct and indirect effects are thus, up to this point, complementary to each other. After passing this point, however, when the plant is possessed of excess of energy, its power of absorbing stimulus appears to undergo diminution. The direct effect of stimulus is then found reduced, and there is no negative after-effect, due to the absorbed component of the external stimulus. From these considerations we are enabled to understand the curious growth-response that was observed in varying the temperature of the plant from 34° C. to 35° C. (fig. 182, Pp. 445). The change of temperature was in that case accomplished, as will be remembered, by changing the intensity of the electrical heating current. The change from 34° C. to 35° C. thus produced was not, however, brought about at once, but took place in the course of a pericd of three minutes. We had consequently the stimulating effect of varzation of tem- perature bringing about contraction, followed by the accele- rated rate of growth which constituted the negative after- effect of that stimulus, A/vs the accelerated rate of growth due to rising temperature. That the first of these two factors played a considerable part in this acceleration, is seen from the fact that the permanent increase in the rate of growth charac- teristic of the higher temperature of 35° C. is smaller than the DETECTION OF LATENT STIMULUS 463 acceleration that precedes it. Incidentally, we see here the difference between the temperature-effect per se, and the stimulating effect of sudden variations of temperature, con- stituting thermal shocks. In the simple case we have just studied, where the whole amount of incident stimulus was expressed in work external and internal, without any loss from molecular friction, the sum total of the two forms of response was found approxi- mately the same under a constant stimulus; but in other cases, where a certain amount of energy is wasted in over- coming molecular sluggishness, the results will be slightly different, for at a temperature below the optimum a por- tion of the stimulus will be wasted in overcoming such sluggishness, whereas near the optimum temperature the loss entailed on this account will be very slight. Hence, the sum of direct and indirect responses, near the optimum, will in such cases be somewhat greater than at a temperature several degrees lower. I give below a table which shows at a glance the direct effect and the indirect after-effect, obtained at the three temperatures of 30° C., 35° C., and 37° C. respectively, with three different specimens, one of which was a rice-seedling (Ovyza sativa), and the two others flower-buds of Crvzmum Lily. Each response given is the mean of three. TABLE SHOWING DIRECT AND INDIRECT EFFECTS AT THREE DIFFERENT TEMPERATURES Memnera| Direct effect Indirect effect | Specimen pi (retardation of (acceleration of | ‘Total effect ture | = | growth) growth) | ali f 30° C. | 15 divisions | 1o0divisions | 25 divisions | eteeseeduns of 4} | 27, C 5 a ae | Oryza sativa | Soe a 22) j 5 ae 23 Bye. |= LO 4¢ | oO a ite) a5 | 1 ee coll awe |e wis 2 ( 30° C. | 32 divisions 13°5 divisions | 45°5 divisions 2. Crinum Lily || 35° C. 38 53 | ters 7 460°5 *s - ( Biya G: 24 ” oO 33 24 99 2 , : | = x ( 30°C. | 18 divisions 17 divisions | 35 divisions ° . —. | ) | 3. Crinum Lily | aE (Co 28 35 13 En | 41 ss Bie 12 = fe) “e 12 at 404 PLANT RESPONSE It will be seen from the readings given by specimens I and 2, that up to the optimum the total response is approxi- mately constant, while at 37° C. there is no expression of energy held latent. The condition of specimen 3 having been at starting somewhat sub-tonic, the total effect is heightened at the optimum, for reasons which have been explained. It has already been said that the proportion of stimulus held latent will be greater with the degree of sub- tonicity of the plant. That this was the condition of specimen 3, then, is demonstrated, not only by the increase of the total effect at the optimum temperature, but also by the fact that at 30° C. so large a proportion of the stimulus as almost exactly one-half is held latent. At 37° C. here, as in the other cases, there was no latent component. Is the change..induced by stimulus always of an explosive character P—It has generally been supposed that stimulus causes response by an explosive chemical change. According to this theory, the stimulus acts as upon a trigger, to release suddenly a large amount of energy previously held latent in the tissue. The response is thus assumed to be always disproportionately larger than the stimulus, and to be brought about by chemical degradation, or dissimilation of the living tissue. The tissue, thus reduced below par, is sup- posed to be restored by the process of assimilation. In Chapter X., however—on Theories concerning Different Types of Response—I adduced considerations, showing that there are cases of responsive phenomena in which this description would not hold good ; that is tosay, there are instances in which the response cannot be due to a chemical down change of explosive character. Nor is it always true that response is disproportionally larger than stimulus. The series of experiments which has just been described offers conclusive evidence on this point, of a quantitative character. Further, if the theory of an explosive down change had been the real explanation of response in general, then it is clear that in the case of the response induced by stimulus in grow- ing organs, recovery would have taken place slowly, and DETECTION OF LATENT STIMULUS 465 would have culminated in the restoration of the original rate of growth; but we actually find, on the contrary, that, immediately following the responsive retardation, there is an acceleration of growth above the normal, and the true recovery, or restoration of the normal rate, takes place only after this. Thus there is here, instead of a run down, an actual increase, of the energy of the system. Again, from this constancy of the sum of the immediate and the after-effects of stimulus, we can see that, as in an inorganic system, so also in a living organism, the law of the Conservation of Energy holds good. For while at the optimum point the entire stimulus finds expression in direct response (stimulus being here equal to response), below the optimum the direct response is less than the stimulus, the missing fraction being left to find expression in the negative after-effect. Relation between stimulus and response in different tonic conditions.—The experiments which I have described were carried out under the thermal form of stimulation, which is, as already explained, the most satisfactory in practice. We shall hereafter come across instances of the after-effect of accelerated growth, as caused by stimulus of light. And a similar after-effect has already been seen to be caused by electrical stimulus (p. 436). But for the clear demonstration of this particular effect, electrical stimulus is not very suit- able, inasmuch as it is apt to induce fatigue and, through electrical polarisation, a certain amount of tissue-change. I shall, however, describe an experiment, using this mode of stimulation, which, by its responsive indications, will demon- strate certain differences in the internal conditions of tissues, according as they are sub- or super-tonic. | I have shown that the general excitability of the tissue at 30° C. is superficially the same as that at 37° C. or thereabouts ; that is to say, the direct responses at these two temperatures are in some cases approximately the same ; but we have seen that there is a difference of molecular condition at these two points, for at 30° C. the tissue is capable of holding a portion HH 466 PLANT RESPONSE of the incident stimulus latent, by which its molecular mobility becomes enhanced, whereas at 37° C. there is already the fullest molecular mobility. If now we apply at 30° C. stimuli which increase, say, in arithmetical progression, we see that by the very reception of the increasing stimuli the tissue is made to approach more and more closely to the optimum condition, at which point, as we have seen, the whole of the stimulus is given up immediately, in the form of direct response. Hence the curve showing the relation between stimulus and response in a tissue that is in a condition below the optimum will be steep, and somewhat convex to the abscissa which represents the stimulus ; but at 37° C., when no molecular sluggishness has to be overcome, we may expect the response to increase proportionately with the stimulus; that is to say, the curve showing the relation between stimulus and response will now tend to be a straight line. In order to apply electrical stimulation whose intensity was increased by known amounts, I used an induction coil, the primary coil of which was completely within the secondary. The ordinary method of Du Bois-Reymond’s sliding coil was not very suitable in this case, because the increasing intensity obtained by sliding the coil inwards is merely qualitative. The required definite increase of induction-shock I secured by suitable augmentations in the value of the current that flowed round the primary coil. In order to determine these values, a preliminary experiment was carried out. A storage battery was in circuit with the primary coil, which had interposed in it also an ammeter and a rheostat. By increasing the resistance, a moderate current, say C, read by the ammeter, was adjusted to flow round the circuit. The secondary circuit contained a ballistic galvanometer, which by its throw indicated the intensity of the induced current at make or break of the primary circuit. When the first current, c, had given an induced current, which caused a deflection of, say, 30, it was increased to C,, when the deflection due to induction was found to be 50; and lastly a third value, or C,,, was found, whose induction-effect was 70. In this manner, DETECTION OF LATENT STIMULUS 467 induction-shocks, increasing in arithmetical progression as 3 to 5 to 7, were determined. Tetanic shocks of the described intensity —3, 5, 7—the total application of each group being a period of 5’, were now given, response being taken after each. In this manner two series of responses were obtained at the temperatures of 30° C. and 37°C. At both temperatures with the stimulus-intensity of 3, the responses given by this particular specimen were the same. But while ey 47-¢; the lower of the: two curves, showing the relation between stimulus and response, is a straight line, that at 30° C., the upper of the two, is seen to ascend more steeply and to exhibit convexity to the abscissa (fig. 186). After-effect.—The phenomenon which is generally referred to as the after-effect is characterised by so Fic. 186. Curve showing the Relation between many complexities as to have been Stimulus and Response regarded as highly perplexing. After in the same Organ, under : : the two different Tonic the foregoing analysis, however, by Conditions of 30° C. (upper . e Ay r r 450 we a which it has been resolved into two see and 37° C. (lower elements, much of this obscurity The abscissa represents the will be found to have disappeared. intensity of stimulus, and < the ordinate the extent of We saw that the first effect induced response. The response by a very strong or long-continued was the same in beth cases ; = : é when the stimulus-intensity external stimulus consisted in bring- was 3; but later, the . : 5 rve for 20° eee ing about the continuation of the cuive (Prisons Geis : : : é to rise more steeply than direct effect itself, its persistence that at 37° C., exhibiting , : : at the same time convexity depending on the intensity and tor the abe duration of that stimulation. And this we have distinguished as the positive after-effect. A com- ponent part of the external stimulus, however, as we also saw, becomes latent, thus increasing the internal energy ; and the expression due to this element—the negative after-effect-—is opposite in sign to the effect of direct stimulus and its positive H H 2 468 PLANT RESPONSE after-effect. Thus a plant under natural conditions, acted upon by different stimuli, gives to each stimulus direct re- sponse, direct after-response, and indirect response. But as all these individual stimuli do not act, or cease to act, simultaneously, we can easily understand the infinite com- plexity of the combinations which take place between the direct and indirect effects of stimuli of unlike forms, whose maxima, instead of being coincident, are superposed on each other, with various differences of phase. Factors which determine periodic after-effects.—We shall now proceed to enumerate some of the most important stimulating factors instrumental in modifying the response of growth. (1) Stémulus of light.—l\f we take the average rate of srowth during the twenty-four hours as the normal, then the direct effect of this stimulus will appear as a retardation of that normal rate of growth, and, after the long-continued action of the whole day’s illumination, this retardation may persist for a time as the positive after-effect. Later, however, on account of the stimulus which has been absorbed and held latent, we shall observe an acceleration of growth above the normal, or a negative after-effect. The persistence, again, of this negative after-effect will depend on the amount of the energy held latent by the tissue. The diurnal sequence of light and darkness will, after Jong repetition, impress itself upon the organism, and, other factors remaining constant, will find expression as periodic retardation and acceleration of growth during day and night ; such periodicity continuing to show itself, for some time, even when the plant is kept in continuous darkness. (2) Temperature—The effect of temperature up to the optimum point will, by increasing the internal energy, prove favourable to growth. Thus, the temperature during day- light will usually be favourable, with the exception of the tropical noon, when it may be excessive. A certain amount of heat, again, may be stored up in the plant to give the after- effect. At night, if the fall of temperature be very great, there will be, relatively to this factor, a retardation of growth. DETECTION OF LATENT STIMULUS 469 (3) Chemical stimulus.—This we see supplied by the salts taken up by the roots, and by the process of photo-synthesis in the leaves. The amount of the former supply is depen- dent not only on the richness of the soil, but also on the suctional activity of the plant ; and the latter on the effective intensity of light. (4) Turgidity—The turgid condition of the plant will depend, firstly, on the supply of water; secondly, on the suctional activity ; and thirdly, on the relative absence of a loss of water by transpiration. We have seen how watering the roots of the plant will cause an immediate response of enhanced growth, and the seasonal periodicity induced by this cause may be observed in a yey striking manner in tropical countries. The suctiona! activity, depending as it does on the internal energy of the plant, will tend to be augmented by an advantageous temperature, and by the after-effect of the absorbed stimulus of light; but the turgid condition will be reduced by active transpiration, which is relatively greater in the daytime. We thus see how numerous are the factors which co- operate to bring about periodic fluctuations in the rate of growth during every twenty-four hours. Of all these factors, the alternation of day and night is the most pronounced in its action. The curve of growth, then, will exhibit not only a large wave of alternation due to the diurnal period, but also a number of sub-waves. And even beyond these, superposed upon them, we may expect, when the magnification is suffi- ciently great, to observe systems of still smaller wavelets, caused by the rhythmicity of growth. Continuous photographic record of the pulsations of Desmodium.—Before describing the periodic diurnal variation of that autonomous response which we know as growth, it occurred to me that a continuous record of another form of autonomous response—that is to say, of the rhythmic pulsations of Desmodium—might prove interesting. I was fortunate enough to succeed in obtaining a very good record A70 PLANT RESPONSE of these pulsations during a period of twelve hours, by means of photography. The record began at 6 P.M. and ended at 6 A.M. During this time there were no fewer than 180 con- stituent pulses, and it will be noticed that these again fall into groupings, whose average period is a little over an hour, there being about ten such groups in the course of twelve hours (fig. 187). Returning now to the question of periodic growth-fluctua- tions, Sachs and others have measured the different rates of growth at various hours within the twenty-four, and from the data thus obtained have constructed curves which showed yf 6 P.M. Q P.M. 12 12 3 A.M. 6 A.M. Fic. 187. Continuous Photographic Record of Autonomous Pulsation of Desmodium gyrans from 6 P.M. to 6 A.M. The lower record is in continuation of the upper. the periodicity of the rate of growth. These curves exhibit the daily period in a marked manner; but the subordinate waves are more or less obliterated, in consequence of the fact that the data from which the curves were constructed were obtained from discontinuous observations. The curves thus deduced show marked differences also, according as. their points were determined frequently or at long intervals. Record of periodic variation of rate of growth.—For this reason it appeared to me important to devise means by which, not the growth, but the varzatzons, of its rate might be automatically recorded, directly and continuously, for any DETECTION OF LATENT STIMULUS Az length of time. The curve thus obtained ought instanta- neously to mark the fluctuations of rate throughout the period in question. This I have been able to accomplish by means of a modified Method of Balance. From the description of that method already given, it will be under- stood that after the establishment of the average balance, if the rate increases, the curve will move upwards. When, after this, the rate of growth returns to the normal, the balance will be re-established, and the curve become again horizontal ; but if the growth at any time should fall below the average, the curve will descend. In this way pe- riodic fluctuations in the rate of growth may be recorded. For the purposes of the modified method of record, the compensating arrange- ment used for balance has to be somewhat altered. In recording these long periodic changes, we have fluctuations of larger am- plitude than those of au- . tonomous pulsation. The spot of light which is thrown from the mirror of Fic. 188. Hydrometric Apparatus for the experimental Optic ees, Seauae Variation of Lever upon the second, or -) float, supporting the plant, which is compensating, mirror is attached by thread to optic lever, L. : Ww, wheel, for adjustment of balancing thus apt to fall outside eet the range of the latter. In order to overcome this difficulty I mount the plant on the float itself, and adjust the outflow of water from the cylinder, so that the upward growth of the plant is, at a given moment, exactly compensated by the descent of the float supporting it. Deviations above or below the balanced rate of growth may then be followed with a recording pen, 472 PLANT RESPONSE or will record themselves on a sensitised photographic film wrapped round the revolving drum. In carrying out such a continuous record, certain pre- cautions are necessary. Owing to transpiration, the float on which the plant is mounted will become lighter, causing an ascensional movement of the record. In order to obviate this, it is only necessary (1) to weight the float to such an extent that the variation caused by transpiration is negli- gible ; and (2), in addition to this, the diameter of the cylin- drical float may be so increased as to reduce still further the ascensional movement due to loss of weight by transpiration. By these means the error from this source may be reduced to any extent desired. In the figure of the apparatus which is here given (fig. 188), the specimen was a young seedling of Oryza sativa, in which transpiration was relatively little. I shall presently give photographic records obtained in the manner described. Continuous photographic re- cord of periodic variations of transpiration. — By a somewhat similar method we are enabled to determine the periodic variation of the rate of transpiration. In this case, the plant is mounted with its roots in a test-tube, which acts like a float, and is partially filled with '1G.189. Photographic Record showing Variation of Rate of Transpiration in Cucur- dita, from 3 P.M. to 12 P.M. The rate is seen to undergo enhancement till about 11.30 P.M., after which there is a rapid fall of the rate of transpiration. water, but not so full as to make it sink. The test-tube containing the plant is attached to one arm of the Optic Lever, and the outflow from the outer cylinder so adjusted that the ascensional movement of the test-tube, due to its loss of weight by transpiration, is exactly balanced by the subsidence of the water-level of the water that buoys it up. In order DETECTION OF LATENT STIMULUS 473 to prevent the loss of water from the cylinder by evaporation, a film of oil covers the surface. In this way I obtained the accompanying record of variation of rate of transpiration in a young specimen of Cucurbita, from 3 P.M. to 12 P.M. (fig. 189). It will be noticed that in this case there is an enhancement of transpiration which continues with a single fluctuation till past 11 P.M., after which there is a sudden depression of the rate. Continuous photographic record of the diurnal varia- tion of the rate_of growth.—I shall first give a photographic record (fig. 190)ftaken from a seedling of Oryza sativa, only Fic. 190. Continuous Photographic Record of Variation of Rate of Growth in Four Days’ Old Seedling of Oryza satzva, from 3 P.M. till 9 A.M., that is during Eighteen Hours four days old. The diurnal periodicity has already, it will be seen, become fairly impressed, though it is not yet sufficiently powerful to mask, to any great extent, the subsidiary periodi- cities induced by other factors. The record, it must be remembered, was taken in continuous darkness, being com- menced at 3 P.M., when it was balanced. From this time to 9 P.M. there were three pulsations. From 6 till after 8 P.M. there was depression of the average rate of growth, after which it rose somewhat rapidly till 12.30 A.M., exhibiting during that period two groups of two pulsations each. There was now a quick fall for the next half-hour, and after this 474 PLANT RESPONSE the growth-rate rose more or less continuously till 8 AM. The srowth-rate then began to fall during the course of the day. ; The second record was taken with a seedling of Zama- rindus indica fourteen days old, the diurnal periodicity being thus deeply impressed. It was placed in the dark room, mounted on the float, and the balanced record begun at 3 P.M. It will be seen that, as the positive after-effect of the day’s illumination, there was a depression of the rate FIG. 191. Continuous Photographic Record of Variation of Rate ot Growth in Seedling of Zamarindus indica, a Fortnight Old, from 3 P.M. to 3 A.M. Owing to positive after-effect of daylight, there is a depression of rate of growth, although the seedling was now placed in a dark room. In the evening, however, the rate began to rise. of growth, though the plant was kept in the dark. This persisted for two hours, till 5 P.M., after which the rate showed increase, there being three pulsations before the end of the record, at 3 A.M. (fig. 191). Annual rings of wood and seasonal periodicity.—The different growths of wood in spring and autumn, leading to the formations known as ‘annual rings, constitute a pheno- menon of growth not yet fully explained. I may here point out an important factor in connection with this subject. It DETECTION OF LATENT STIMULUS A75 has already been demonstrated that growth is a phenomenon of excitatory reaction, being at its maximum when the excitability of the tissue is greatest. Thus varying expres- sions of growth at the two seasons, as seen in the production of different sized cells during spring and autumn, would appear natural, if they could be correlated to differences of excitability, characteristic of those seasons. Now all modes of testing degrees of excitability lead to the conclusion that while it is very great in spring and summer, it is very much enfeebled in autumn and winter. Thus, in the latter season, contractility under stimulation, velocity of transmission of excitation, and the electrical response of a tissue, are all found to undergo a marked diminution, as compared with spring and summer. SUMMARY On the cessation of strong stimulus the responsive movement continues for a time in the same direction. This is the foszzzve after-effect. A portion of the incident stimulus is absorbed and held latent, thus increasing the latent energy of the plant. On the cessation of stimulus this latent component, either immediately or after a time, finds expression in an opposite responsive movement. ‘This is the wegatzve after-effect. In the case of growth-response, the positive after-effect consists in the persistence for a time of the retardation of growth, and the negative after-effect exhibits itself as an acceleration of the rate of growth above the normal. With moderate stimulus and under normal conditions the sum of the direct effect and the negative after-effect (due to the latent component) remains constant up to the optimum; that is to say, the sum of the external work (direct effect) and internal work (negative after-effect) done by the stimulus is the same. The direct and the negative after-effect of stimulus are thus complementary. At the exact optimum almost the whole stimulus will find expression in direct response, there being little or no 476 PLANT RESPONSE latent component. In a sub-tonic condition, on the other hand, a greater proportion of the stimulus is temporarily held latent, and expresses itself as the negative after-effect, the direct responses being here correspondingly diminished. Above the optimum there is no latent component, and the general receptivity of the organ shows great diminution. From the constancy of the sum of the direct and indirect effects it is demonstrated that, with regard to some forms of response at least, response is not disproportionately greater than stimulus. Thus the theory that response must always be due to an explosive chemical change does not hold good. The curve showing the relation between stimulus and response is appropriately modified by the tonic condition of the tissue. As each stimulus of every form thus finds expression in direct response, direct after-effect, and indirect after-effect, and as there are many forms of stimulus which under natural conditions act on the plant, whose maxima, instead of being coincident, are superposed on each other, in varying differ- ences of phase, highly complex periodicities are induced, and find expression in the various forms of plant response. Among such varying factors of stimulation may be mentioned the diurnal alternation of light, temperature, chemical stimulus, and varying turgescence. The induced periodicities which result from the conditions described may be seen in the periodic groupings which appear inacontinuous record of the autonomous pulsations of Desmodium for example. The autonomous response of growth, as the result of the periodically acting stimuli mentioned, exhibits not only a large wave of alternation, due to the diurnal period, but also a number of sub-waves. But the impression made on the organism by the diurnal period is the deepest of these, and tends in an old plant to subordinate all others in a marked degree. In the responses of a seedling a few days old, how- ever, the minor waves are very distinct. CHAPTER 2X V AN INVESTIGATION INTO THE DIFFERENT EFFECTS OF DRUGS ON PLANTS OF DIFFERENT ‘CONSTITUTIONS’ General consideration of the problem---‘ Constitution’ and the elements which determine it—Methods of investigation-—Action of carbonic acid gas—Action of ether—Effect of solution of sodium carbonate—Effect of solution of sugar— Effect of alcohol—FEffect of acids—Effect of alkali—Antagonistic action of alkalis and acids—Action of strong solution of sodium chloride—Effect of poisonous solution of copper sulphate— Opposite effects of the same dose on different constitutions—Opposite effects of large and small doses. WE have already studied the effect of various chemical agenis on the physiological condition of plants, as seen from the modifications induced by them in ordinary and in autonomous responses. We have also noticed the remark- able similarity between these effects in the two cases of plant and animal tissues, and I have drawn attention to the great practical utility of these investigations, inasmuch as the experiments carried out on plants may be made to throw light on many obscure phenomena regarding the effects of drugs on the animal system. One puzzling fact, however, which is encountered in medical practice, is, that the same drug will often produce varying effects on different individuals; and this is vaguely ascribed to dis- similarity of ‘ constitution.’ Similarly, we have sometimes seen different effects to occur in plants under the action of a single given drug. A large dose of some depressing agent, for example, though it will ultimately produce the depressing effect, will not always do so at once, for in some cases there will be a pre- liminary period of exaltation of response. The same toxic dose, again, which will in some instances kill the plant, will 478 PLANT RESPONSE in others fail to do so, the plant being ultimately able to shake off the depressing influence after an interval of struggle. In studying suctional response, again, we found that copper sulphate applied at the roots induced, in some cases, an immediate arrest of suction, while in other instances this arrest did not take place till aftera long time. The depression of suction which was induced by the application of strong sodium chloride, again, was in some cases imme- diate, and in others preceded by a fairly long period of an exalted rate of suction (pp. 384, 385). All these variations of results we regarded as due to individual differences of constitution, or of tonic condition. Thus we can only hope to arrive at a complete knowledge regarding the action of any given drug if we first obtain a precise understanding of what is meant by constitution, and of how, for experimental purposes, specimens of a definite characteristic constitution can be secured, while others can be subjected to an ascertained variation in a pre-determined manner. It will thus be made possible to study the effect of a drug, by applying it (1) in solutions of different strengths to a number of plants of identical constitution ; and (2) ina single strength of solution to specimens of definitely varying constitutions. ‘Constitution’ and the elements which determine it.— The first factor in determining constitution will consist of those properties which have been impressed upon the plant by its heredity. The second will depend upon its environment. The sum total of the energy absorbed by the plant from its surroundings we have already designated as the tonic con- dition. Itis clear that we may secure the factor of a constant heredity by taking either seedlings from the same batch of seeds, or organs from the same plant. These, again, when maintained under the same environmental conditions, in respect of temperature and other circumstances, will give us plants having practically the same constitution. In order, next, to obtain specimens of different but well-ascertained constitutions, it is only necessary to keep these under known —- - - --—-—-——s EFFECTS OF DRUGS ON PLANTS 479 differences of tonic condition. Thus, all other factors being maintained constant, we may have three clearly defined states, in the case, say, of Crvzxum Lily, according as it is kept at a temperature of 30° C., 34° C., or 37° C. The first of these we may regard as the normal; the second as near the optimum; and the third as intermediate between optimum and maximum. The excitability of the plant kept at the optimum will be the greatest ; but though the excita- bilities of those at 30° C. and 37° C. will be approximately the same, yet, in the latter case, the plant will possess an excess of latent energy which will be wanting in the former. Having thus secured these definite artificial constitutions of different values, some of the investigations given at the end of this chapter will show how free from uncertainty the action of drugs may be made, and how rational an expla- nation can be given of the observed variations of effect. Methods of investigation.—I shall now proceed to describe the general methods of experiment, in studying the effects of drugs, from the modifications which they induce in growth-response. There are two ways of doing this. Accord- ing to the first, we take a record of the growth before and after the application of the reagent. From the variation then seen in the rate of growth the excitatory or depressing nature of the drug may be ascertained. The second, or Method of the Balanced Crescograph, is much more delicate ; it exhibits each transient variation of response, and its time- relations, with perfect clearness. The balanced horizontal record, which is first taken, indicates the normal rate of growth. A deviation upwards from this horizontal line will indicate accelerated growth; a return to the horizontal will mean a regaining of the normal rate ; and a deviation down- wards will show responsive retardation. The specimens used for this investigation were Cvzzum Lilies, and, unless stated to the contrary, the experiments were carried out at the normal temperature of 30° C. Action of carbonic acid gas.—I first give a record of the effect of carbonic acid gas on growth (fig. 192), taken by 480 PLANT RESPONSE the Unbalanced Method. The normal rate of growth was ‘c0o6 mm. per minute. On passing CO, into the plant chamber the immediate effect was an acceleration, the rate for the next five minutes being ‘oo9 mm. or I} times the normal. Under the continued action of carbonic acid, how- ever, the rate underwent a rapid diminution, and, as is seen by the slope of the curve becoming horizontal, growth was arrested fifteen minutes after the introduction of carbonic acid into the plant chamber. On the re-intro- duction of fresh air the growth was slowly renewed, and gradually returned to _ its Fic. 192. Effect of Carbonic Acid original rate. The effect of Gas on Growth carbonic acid on _ growth- The first arrow indicates introduction response, then, is a prelimi- of CO,, which induces preliminary ; enhancement of growth, followed nary exaltation, followed by by subsequent arrest, The second depression and arrest, which arrow indicates the introduction of fresh air, followed by the revival arrest, if the action be not too of erent. Recess taken by Un Jong continued, proves to be only temporary. Action of ether.—This experiment shows the curious difference of results which occurs, according as an applica- tion is external or internal. In this and in the following cases, with the exception of the experiments on acids and alkalis, I shall use the Balanced Method, as this brings out even transient variations in a very striking manner. The balanced horizontal record is seen to the left of each figure. In curve a of fig. 193 is shown the effect of an external appli- cation of this reagent, ether vapour being introduced into the plant chamber. It will be seen that there was an imme- diate retardation of growth, which lasted for more than a minute. This was followed by an acceleration of growth, which lasted for two minutes. There was then a depression, which continued, and culminated in the arrest of growth. EFFECTS OF DRUGS ON PLANTS 481 In 4 is shown the effect of an zxternal application, made by replacing the water, by which the cut end of the stem was normally surrounded, with a 3 per cent. solution of ether. It will be seen that, as the specimen sucked up the solution, the immediate effect of this internal application was an enhancement of the rate of growth, and that this was followed by depression, lead- ing to arrest of growth. The preliminary depression seen in @ is thus wanting in the Fic. 193. Balanced Records otf case of the internal applica- Effect of Ether on Growth. Up : ; Curves represent Acceleration tion. It might be thought Above, and Down Curves Re- that this first depression, as tardation Below, the Normal seen in the case of the ex- a Effect of external application, and Boni ‘i 6 of internal application of ether. ternal application, was due to Successive thick dots in the base- 5 . , line indicate time in minutes in 3 slight cooling, caused by this and following records. Arrow the introduction of ether. We shows moment of application. know, however, that while lowering of the temperature, as long as that is below the optimum, would suffice to retard growth, a similar lowering of it when above the optimum would have the opposite effect of acceleration. Now I find that this preliminary retardation of growth, seen in fig. 193, a, takes place in exactly the same manner when the experiment is repeated with the specimen at 38°C. It cannot, there- fore, be due to the suggested cooling, which must in any case, under the fy, ro4q. Excitatory Effect experimental conditions, have been of Dilute Solution of So- : ‘ dium Carbonate on Growth extremely slight. There is, however, another explanation, which more nearly meets the require- ments of the case. We know that any sudden variation of environmental conditions is apt, generally speaking, to act as a stimulus, and the effect of direct stimulation is always to id! 482 PLANT RESPONSE induce contraction, which would in the present case take the form of a transient retardation of growth. Effect of solution of sodium carbonate.—In the course of the following experiments the chemical reagents are administered internally, by applying the solution at the cut end of the specimen. A dilute solution of sodium carbonate is known to increase excitability in the case of animal tissues. I find that this holds good in the case of the growth-response of plants, growth being accelerated, as will be seen in fig. 194, where the balanced record suddenly gives place to an ascending curve. But in the case of chemical reagents in general, and of this especially, it must be remembered that the strength of the solution, or dose, is an important element in the result. A ‘5 per cent. solution of sodium carbonate was always found in these experiments to be an excitant ; but as the strength of the solution was increased, the excita- tory effect was found to be gradually diminished, until at 2 per cent. it became neutral. If now the strength were still further increased, say to 5 per cent., the effect was an actual depression of the rate of growth. Effect of solution of sugar.—I next give a record (fig. 195) showing the effect of the application of sugar Fic. 195. Acceleration of jin a 2 per cent. solution. This is Growth by Application of : : Solution of Sugar seen to induce a responsive accelera- tion of growth. In the case of the Crinum Lily, this acceleration is found to occur even under a 5 per cent. solution of sugar; but very much stronger solu- tions induce depression. Effect of alcohol. We have _ hitherto observed different reagents inducing a more or less uniform acceleration or depression. In the case of alcohol, given in 5 per cent. solution, however, we obtain a very curious instance of alternating spasmodic effects (fig. 196); that is to say, the growth at one moment exhibits a sudden acceleration, and EFFECTS OF DRUGS ON PLANTS 483 at the next a sudden depression, such alternations being continued for a considerable length of time. On repeating the experiment at the higher temperature of 34° C. I found that these spasmodic alterna- tions became still more violent— that is to say, of greater ampli- tude, though less frequent. At a much higher temperature, how- ever, the effect of alcohol was an immediate depression. Still stronger solutions caused arrest FIG. 196. Spasmodic Alternations of Growth under Alcohol of growth at all temperatures. Effect of acids.—We found, in the case of the autono- mous responses of Desmodium, as of cardiac muscle, that acids induced relaxation, and that the long-continued action of such a reagent, or a strong solution, would bring about arrest in the relaxed position. Curiously enough, then, in the case of growth, which I have shown to be an instance of multiple or autono- mous response, I find an effect exactly parallel. In order to hasten the result I used a somewhat strong solution, namely 4 per cent., of hydrochloric acid. This, as will be seen (fig. 197), caused a marked relaxation, and growth came to a standstill some six minutes after- wards. In this experiment, and the F!¢. 197. Unbalanced J ‘ i : Record showing Ac- following on the effect of alkali, the tion of Acid in Causing Relaxation and Ulti- _ se ae ee record was taken under unbalanced dee Aa ce oa conditions. Effect of alkali.—The effect of alkali in the case of Des- modtum, and also of cardiac muscle, is to produce arrest in the contracted position ; a similar effect is strikingly exhi- bited in the growth-record given in fig. 198. The first part of this record shows the normal rate of growth. A 3 per cent. solution of sodium hydrate was then applied at the point marked with the downward arrow (+). It will be noticed LZ 484 PLANT RESPONSE that this induces a very great contraction, and that in the course of seven minutes there occurs an arrest of growth, in a contracted position. Fic. 198. Unbalanced Record showing the Action of Al- kali, and the Antagonistic Action of Subsequent Appli- cation of Acid The downward arrow (1) indi- cates application of alkali, which induces arrest of growth in contracted posi- tion. The upward arrow (*) indicates application of acid, which by its antagonistic action renews growth. It willalso be seen that the specimen, after the application, actually be- came shorter than it had been before. The alkali therefore had the effect not merely of arresting growth, but also of causing an active contraction of the tissue. Antagonistic action of alkalis and acids.—We saw that in the autonomous of Des- modium and cardiac muscle, the state of standstill induced by the action of either acid or alkali was neutralised and counteracted by the antagonistic action of the other (cf. fig.*155). Ihave detected precisely the same peculiarity in the case of growth-response also. responses It was seen in the course of the last experiment that growth was brought to astate of standstill, in the contracted position, by the action FIG. 199. Effect of Strong Solution of NaCl on Rate of Growth, as Modified by Different Constitutions of Specimens At a temperature of 30° C. there is an immediate depression. ] 3 Near the optimum there is a well-marked resistance, and preliminary acceleration before depression sets in. These records were taken under balanced conditions. The same is true to a less extent at 37° C. At 44° C. there was normally no growth, but this was temporarily initiated under the action of strong NaCl solution. of alkali (fig. 198). Record taken by Method of Balance. At this point, as marked by the upward . arrow (+), acid was applied. This reagent now had the effect EFFECTS OF? DRUGS ON PLANTS 485 as will be seen from the record, of neutralising the previous contractile arrest, and in the course of two minutes it had brought about the renewal of growth. Action of strong solution of sodium chloride.—In the course of the investigation on suctional response we noticed that the effect of this reagent, when applied to the root, was not always to cause a diminution of suction, as might have been expected had osmotic action been the only factor. This reagent, on the contrary, usually operated to bring about a preliminary acceleration of suction. And I have already ex- plained that this was due to its excitatory character, acting in opposition to the osmotic action set up by the strong solu- tion. It is usually supposed that the excitatory effect of strong solution of salt on animal tissues is due to the osmotic withdrawal of water; but we have here seen that similar excitation is induced in vegetable tissues without any with- drawal of water. Hence the usual theory of the action of salt solution in causing excitation is rendered very doubt- ful. We found, in fact, that when the tonic condition of the tissue was favourable, the excitatory reaction predominated, and there was a consequent enhancement of suctional activity. In other instances, where the tonic condition was less favourable, osmotic action predominated, and there was a consequent depression of the normal rate of suction (p. 385). It is thus seen that the factor of variation in these two different cases was the constitution, or tonic condition, of the plant ; we are now able to study with precision the influence exercised by constitution, in causing the plant to struggle against, or succumb to, the action of adverse external cir- cumstances. The action, in causing variation of suction, of a strong solution of salt, applied internally through the cut end of the stem, can also be studied by means of growth-response ; for while increased suctional activity will give rise to a posi- tive turgidity-variation, with concomitant enhancement of the rate of growth, diminished suctional activity will have the opposite effect, of retarding the rate of growth. We shall next observe the effect of this reagent on different specimens 486 PLANT RESPONSE of Crinum Lily, which had been taken from the same flower- head, and in which different constitutions were artificially induced by carrying out the experiments at 30° C., 34°C, 37° C., and 44° C. The records were taken under balanced conditions. On making an application of a solution so strong as 10 per cent. to a specimen at the normal temperature of 30° C., the result was always a depression which set in immediately ; but when the tonic condition of the plant was exalted, by raising the temperature to a point near the optimum—that is to say, to 34° C—the same reagent was found to induce an excitatory effect, its depressing action being postponed for a considerable length of time. The effect obtained at the temperature of 37° C. was very instructive. Responsive excitability at this temperature has been shown to be almost the same, if not indeed slightly lower, than that at 30° C. But we should remember that in these two cases we have very different histories ; for if in the former—that is, at 30° C.— we have a normal amount of latent energy, then it is clear that in the latter—that is to say, at 37° C.—there must be an excess of latent energy. And as the result of this we see that, while at 30° C. the growth-response showed immediate depression, at 37° C. it offered a considerable resistance, as seen in the temporary exaltation of response. Still more interesting, however, was the effect at 44°C. At this tem- perature it will be remembered that there was an apparent arrest of growth, often supposed to be due to the setting-in of heat-rigor. I have shown, however, that, so far from this being the case, there is still at 44° C. a good deal of rhythmic activity, the cessation of growth being due to the fact that in the multiple response of growth each constituent response and recovery had become equal. ‘The presence of this activity at 44° C. becomes quite clear, when we find that the application of salt at this temperature has the effect of renewing for a time the resultant growth which had been in abeyance (fig. 199). Effect of poisonous solution of copper sulphate.—The influence of constitution in determining resistance to adverse EFFECTS OF DRUGS ON PLANTS 487 circumstances is made still more striking when we observe the action on the plant of strongly poisonous reagents, such as 5 per cent. solution of copper sulphate. At the normal temperature of 30° C. its application, as will be seen in the record (fig. 200), induces a very rapid depression, which soon culminates in permanent areest.. But at 34° C.. the resistance offered is consider- able, actually exhibiting it- self in temporary exaltation. At 44° C., again, we observe Fic. 200. The Effect of Different Constitutions in Determining the the poisonous reagent actually Resistance Offered to Poisons. 5 Loe we = ; The Action of 5 per Cent. Solution initiating growth, as in the of Copper Sulphate last case. Opposite effects of the same dose on different consti- tutions.—In the case last considered, we studied the pheno- menon of the resistance offered by the plant to a toxic dose largely in excess of the fatal amount. In spite of the excessive dose we found that, when a favourable constitution was artificially induced, the plant succumbed, it is true, but only after considerable struggle. I shall now proceed to show how, when the artificial constitution is suf- ficiently favourable, the plant, instead of succumbing to an ordinarily fatal dose, can shake off the effect, and may even be stimu- : Fic. 201. The Effect of Favour- lated by it. Thus I found that a able Induced Constitution in Be per. cent. solution of copper Enabling Plant to Shake off : : Result of Toxic Dose of sulphate induces depression and Copper Sulphate : is ultimately fatal at 30° C.; but when the same dose is applied to a plant at 34° C. the effect is seen ina marked exaltation (fig. 201), which continues for a fairly long period, after which it shakes off the effect of the poison altogether, and resumes its normal rate of growth. 488 PLANT RESPONSE Opposite effects of large and small doses.—I have shown the toxic effect of a I per cent. solution of copper sulphate at the normal temperature of 30° C. If, however, the dose be reduced to ‘2 per cent. we shall find that its action becomes stimulatory (fg. 202). This is an interesting illustration of the general fact that a poisonous reagent, if given in sufficiently minute doses, will act as an excitant. In conclusion, a survey of the effects of drugs, both stimulatory and poisonous, reveals the striking fact that the difference between them is a question of quantity. . Sugar, for instance, which is stimulat- ing when given in solutions of, say, I to 5 per cent., becomes depressing when the solution is very strong. Copper sul- Fic. 202. Opposite Effects of phate again, which is regarded Large and Small Doses of . : =e Pe as a poison, is only so at I per A solution of I per cent. copper cent. and upwards, a solution of aad deal bat ‘2 per ceot Pet cont Oe exercises stimulating action. stimulant. The difference be- tween sugar and copper sulphate is here seen to lie in the fact that in the latter case the range of safety is very narrow. Another fact which must be borne in mind in this connection is that a substance like sugar is used by the plant for general metabolic processes, and thus removed from the sphere of action. Thus continuous absorp- tion of sugar could not for a long time bring about sufficient accumulation to cause depression. With copper sulphate, however, the case is different. Here, the constant absorption of the sub-toxic stimulatory dose would cause accumulation in the system, and thus ultimately bring about the death of the plant. SUMMARY Chemical reagents are found to have the same effect on the response of growth as on ordinary response, or as on the autonomous response of Desmodium gyrans. EFFECTS OF DRUGS ON PLANTS 489 Carbonic acid gas induces a preliminary acceleration, followed by retardation and arrest of growth. On _ re- admission of fresh air growth is revived. Ether, when applied internally, causes a preliminary acceleration, followed by retardation and arrest of growth. Minor differences of effect may be observed if the applica- tion be made externally. A very dilute solution of sodium carbonate is an exci- tant, and accelerates growth; but stronger solutions cause retardation. Solution of sugar also stimulates growth, if the strength of solution be not excessive. Alcohol causes spasmodic alternations of growth. Too strong a solution arrests growth. As in the case of autonomous response of Desmodium, so in the response of growth also, acids and alkalis are found to have antagonistic effects. Acid causes relaxa- tion and ultimate arrest of growth; alkali causes arrest of growth in the contracted position. The arrest brought about by one of these agents may be counteracted by the other. The reaction of a specimen to a chemical reagent is determined by the strength of the dose, and by the duration of application. A stimulating agent if given in too strong a dose causes depression. A poisonous reagent, again, if given in a suffi- ciently small dose, acts as an excitant. A clear insight into the nature of ‘constitution, so called, as a factor in determining the reaction of a specimen to a given drug, is afforded by the induction of definite arti- ficial constitutions. This may be carried out by subject- ing the plant to four different typical temperature con- ditions—the ordinary, the optimum, post-optimum, and maximum. It is then found that a plant which has been made to acquire excess of internal energy can struggle against, or even overcome, the influence of such adverse circumstances as 490 PLANT RESPONSE the action of poison, whereas under ordinary circumstances it quickly succumbs. The nature of the response is thus seen to be determined in a definite manner by the constitution, or tonic condition, of the plant, this factor being in its turn dependent on the sum total of the energy which is latent in the organism. Peek Lo VET GEOTROPISM, CHEMOTROPISM AND GALVANOTROPISM CHAPTER XXXVI THE RESPONSIVE CURVATURES CAUSED BY GRAVITY. NEGATIVE GEOTROPISM Statement of the problem of apogeotropic response—Mode in which stimulation is brought about : radial-pressure theory, and theory of statoliths— Mechanics of responsive movement—Experiment demonstrating responsive curvature as brought about by unilateral pressure of particles—Record of curvature induced by gravitation—Record of different rates of curvature when specimen is held at angles of 45° and 135° to the vertical— Determination of the true character of apogeotropic response—Responsive curvature of acellular organs—Curvature of grass haulm under gravity—Growth of grass haulm on a klinostat. IT is well known that growing organs exhibit certain directive movements under the influence of gravitation. Horizontally laid shoots, for example, bend upwards, or against the direction of gravity; while roots react in pre- cisely the opposite way—that is to say, they bend so as to lie in the direction of the force of gravity. In the case of the various forms of stimulation hitherto studied, the action of the plant is well defined and intelligible, consisting of con- cavity of the excited side, in response to a stimulus which is clearly understood. But in the case of geotropism the mode in which stimulation is brought about is not quite evident, and we find, moreover, two directly opposed effects brought about by apparently the same force of gravity: in the root, as already said, a positive movement, that is to say a movement in the direction of gravity ; and in the shoot a negative move- ment, away from the direction of gravity. The seeming impossibility of explaining effects so divergent as due to a single common cause, has led to the modern idea that these responsive movements are ‘executed at the suggestion of changes in the environment, not as the direct 494 PLANT RESPONSE and necessary result of such changes ;’ or in other words that ‘light and gravitation could be classed together as external agencies acting, not directly, but in some unknown indirect manner.’ ' There are two distinct points to be borne in mind in connection with the effect of gravity: first, the question as to how gravity exercises stimulation ; and secondly, that of how, in answer to this stimulus, a definite responsive curvature is induced. Mode in which stimulation is brought about.—Now it is clear that, as regards the former of these points, the only conceivable way in which gravity could produce stimulation is by the effect of weight ; a not, that is to say, by the weight of the plant as a whole, c D 4 but by the differential effect of weight in the cells. Two B A| important theories have been B advanced which offer very D : ‘ ; rational explanations of the Fic. 203. Diagrammatic Representa- 2 ‘ tion showing Differential Effect © means by which gravi-percep- Teio < if : a . a ae on Lateral Walls of tion may be induced. Accord- In the figure to the right the cell is ing to these, the nee laid horizontally, and the lateral differential weight-effect may wall, D, has to bear greater weight 5 than c (after Francis Darwin). be due to the weight of the cell-contents, whether of the sap itself, or of those heavy particles like starch-grains which are contained in it. When, therefore, the cell is laid horizon- tally, it is the lower tangential wall which has to support the relatively greater weight (fig. 203). This theory of hydrostatic or radial pressure was suggested by Pfeffer, and supported by Czapek. The other theory, of s¢atoliths—advocated by Noll, Haberlandt, and Neméc-~-substitutes for the weight of the ' A luminous 7éswmé of our present state of knowledge on -this subject, with all its difficulties and obscurities, is contained in the addresses of Francis Darwin delivered before the British Association in 1891 and 1904. From these I have made the quotations which appear in the text. Figs. 203, 204, and 209 appear as illustrations of the latter address in Va/ure of September 8, 1904. RESPONSIVE CURVATURES—NEGATIVE GEOTROPISM 495 water-column the weight of certain relatively heavy bodies, such as starch-grains, differentially exercised upon the lower tangential walls. In the case of multicellular plants, laid hori- zontally (fig. 204), EE and E’E’ may be regarded as regions in which stimulation is caused by the weight of the particles. The effects produced on the upper and _ lower a ® halves are evidently an- tagonistic, and in spite of this we obtain in the case piste alles aisles Selecta nal. E Mi of shoots a resultant cur- vature upwards, This E! oeee ap 200 ale eo0e ale e@eo0o E! shows that the stimulation 4, c of one half must be greater — than that of the other. Vic. 204. Diagrammatic Representation of a Multicellular Organ The inequality must be due to this difference, that the statoliths in EE rest on the inner, and in E’E’ on the outer, tangential wall. It would thus appear that one of these must be less excitable than the other. Mechanics of responsive movement.—From_ these hydrostatic or statolithic differences of weight, bearing on the ectoplasm of the cell, it is understood that gravi-perception arises, that is to say that the plant perceives the direction of gravity. But there is no explanation as to how stimulation is produced ; nor is there any satisfactory explanation as to the mechanics of the responsive curvature. It is generally supposed, as has been said, that this curvature is not the direct and necessary result of some environmental change, but that it is an instance ‘of a plant reading a signal and directing its growth’ accordingly. It is supposed further that in an apogeotropic cell the curvature takes place by the development ‘of relatively accelerated growth on the side on which the pressure is greatest.’ This view, that the curvature induced in some unknown way ina geotropic organ, by gravitation, depends upon the On the upper side the statoliths act on the inner, and on the lower side on the outer, tangential wall Darwin). (after Francis 496 PLANT RESPONSE accelerated growth of the convex side, is apparently supported by Elfving’s experiments on grass haulms in which growth had been at standstill. ‘He found that the pulvini of grass haulms placed on the klinostat increase in length. This experiment shows incidentally that the klinostat does not remove but merely distributes equally the geotropic stimulus ; also that geotropic stimulus leads to increased, not to diminished, growth. The same thing is proved by the simple fact that a grass haulm shows no growth in its pulvinus while it is vertical, so that when curvature begins (on its being placed horizontally) it must be due to acceleration on the convex, since there is no growth on the concave side in which retardation could occur.’ ! In the cases of response to stimulation, hitherto studied, however, it will be remembered that the fundamental effect was always a contraction and concomitant retardation of srowth, the expansion and acceleration of growth being always a secondary and indirect effect ; but in the case of the response to stimulus of gravity, the interpretation of results which has just been quoted would make it appear that the responsive action brought about by gravity was essentially distinct in its character from the responsive re- action with which we have hitherto been familiar. Experiment demonstrating responsive curvature as brought about by unilateral pressure of particles.—My own object in the course of the present chapter is, however, to demonstrate the fact that the curvature of an apogeotropic plant-organ in response to gravitation is the result of ‘the direct effect of stimulus, its reactive peculiarities being in no way different from those other instances of the response of the plant to stimulation, which we have already studied And in order to simplify my explanation, I shall here ' B.A. Report, 1891, p. 671. It ought to be mentioned here that in other plants when placed on the klinostat this increased rectilinear growth was not observed, leading to the supposition that in such cases a simultaneous increase and decrease of growth-rate on opposite sides of the rotating plant is produced. lbhid. ; Nature, a | To RESPONSIVE CURVATURES—NEGATIVE GEOTROPISM 497 describe a typical experiment showing the excitatory effect of pressure in inducing responsive curvature. That unilateral pressure or contact does induce curvature is known in the case of tendrils; but in the present experiment I shall show that this responsive reaction is not peculiar to tendrils alone, but is exhibited by all organs alike. I was also desirous of making the action on my experimental specimens in every way parallel to that of gravitationally excited tissue in which the statolithic particles exert their weight on a particular F side of the responsive organ. I took a specimen of Crinum Lily and mounted it vertically. I next took a thin strip of india-rubber, the inner surface of which was studded with iron particles, adhering by means of shellac varnish. This was adjusted laterally, on one side of its zone of FIG. 205. Diagrammatic sentation of Experiment showing Repre- growth, so as almost, but not quite, to touch the specimen. On the opposite side was Curvature Induced by Unilateral Pressure Exerted by Particles flower-bud of Cyrzzz272 3 Ss, india- rubber strip studded with iron particies attracted by electro- magnet, M, causing unilateral pressure on growing region; I, index attached to flower. placed an_ electro-magnet, which when excited attracted the strip to which the iron particles adhered, and thus produced a unilateral pressure on the specimen, the magnetic particles functioning as so many statoliths (fig. 205). A recording microscope, which will be fully described in a later chapter, was now focussed on the index, I, attached to the specimen. Before the application of pressure, the quiescent condition of the specimen had been ascertained by noting the stationary position of the index in the field of view of the microscope. On now applying unilateral pressure by exciting the magnet, we might expect to obtain two different effects. The first of KK 498 PLANT RESPONSE these would be the sudden magnetic pull which would cause a movement of the flower away from the pressed side. This effect would be instantaneous, and quickly come to a stop. The second would be the excitatory effect on the specimen of the unilateral irritation caused by the pressure of the iron particles. If this response, like response in general, were to take the form of a contraction, a curvature concave to the source of stimulus would be produced, by which the flower should be seen to bend ¢owards the stimulated side. This stimulatory effect, unlike the mechanical disturbance, would go on increasing with time. In this way it is easy to demonstrate that unilateral irritation of pressure of particles induces contraction, which in growing organs retards the normal rate of growth, the side acted upon thus becoming concave (fig. 206). It should also be borne in mind that in consequence of : ; i ; ive con- Fic. 206. Record of Responsive Curvature this f respons induced in Crzzwm under Experimental traction some of the Conditions shown in fg. 205. Magni: expelled water finds its way to the opposite side, thus increasing its turgidity, with consequent acceleration of rate of growth and convexity of that side. The experi- mental proof of this latter phenomenon will be given in the next chapter. Moreover, in consequence of the curvature induced by this contraction of the excited side, the further side will be stretched, and tension is, as we know, an agent tending to increase the rate of growth. ‘Thus we see that in this case the contraction of the excited side is the acézve factor,and the convexity of the further side is the secondary or subsidiary effect in the growth-curvature. If then such stimulation by unilateral weight-effect, whether statolithic or hydrostatic, be the cause of the apogeotropic curvature induced by gravitation, it follows that the active factor in the process lies in the contraction of the upper side, RESPONSIVE CURVATURES—NEGATIVE GEOTROPISM 499 and that the expansion and convexity of the lower must be merely the subsidiary effect. But it is usually supposed, as we have seen, that the predominatingly active factor in these gravitation-curvatures is the accelerated growth of the convex side. Hence the crucial experiment by which the correctness of one hypothesis or the other may be determined will evidently consist in demonstrating which of the two, contrac- tion or expansion, is actually the essential element in the responsive growth-curvature. And if, further, we should succeed in proving that contraction was the essential element, we should then have established a unity, as between the phenomena of response to gravitation and those which are the results of other forms of stimulation. But before I describe this particular investigation I must explain the method of continuous record, which I employ for observing gravitational curvatures. Record of curvature induced by gravitation.— We have in the Optical Lever a means by which the responsive effect of gravitation-curvature and its variations may be recorded quickly and continuously, and with as great a magnification as is desirable. The horizontally laid specimen, say the scape of Uriclis Lily, has its terminal upper end attached to one arm of the Lever, the other arm being weighted witha slight counter- poise. A continuous record is then taken, on a revolving drum, from which we obtain the responsive curvature and its time-relations. It will be seen from the record that the scape first bent down during a period of forty minutes, after which the effect of gravitation was seen by the reversal of the curve, which indicated the curving up proper to gravitation (fig. 207). It is sometimes thought that this preliminary lapse of time before the appearance of the gravitation-effect, some- times known as the presentation time, is the interval necessary for the statoliths to fall; but I shall presently describe some experiments which will show that the time taken for variation in response to the varying action of gravitation is very much shorter, probably less than a minute. In the present case we K kK 2 500 PLANT RESPONSE must remember that the movement in response to gravitation has to overcome opposite mechanical movements before it can be made perceptible at all; for we have, firstly, the weight of the organ, which tends to make it bend down ; and secondly, on account of this bending, we have greater tension on the upper surface, which, as we have seen, increases the Fic. 207. Record of Apogeotropic Response in Scape of Uriclzs Lily The up curvature due to apogeotropic action proper commenced forty minutes after the specimen was laid horizontally. rate of growth, tending to make that side convex. The differential effect due to gravity has to overcome all this, and becomes visible only when it has done so. After this stage has been reached, the rate of movement upwards goes on increasing until a fairly constant rate is attained. Record of different rates when specimen is held at angles of 45° and 135°.—Czapek has found that the effective stimulus of gravitation is greater when the organ is held at 13%,°,to the vertical than when held at 45°. This difference of effect can be obtained quantitatively with great accuracy by taking successive records with the specimen in the two positions ; and in order to eliminate the effect of any chance disturbance, or of spontaneous variation, I took four alternate records, first with a specimen at 45°, then at 135°, then back once more at 45°, and then again at 135°, each position being maintained just long enough for the attainment of the permanent effect. The change from one position to the other was made quickly, in the course of less than a minute. The experiment was carried out with the unopened flower of Crinum Lily, in which we have already found that the rate of growth is very regular and considerable. RESPONSIVE CURVATURES—NEGATIVE GEOTROPISM 50! The first record was taken after the curvature movement, due to gravitation at an angle of 45°, had attained a constant value. The tip of the flower was then found to be moving at a rate of four divisions per minute. On changing the angle to 135°, the rate of movement showed an imme- diate increase, and attained in the course of five minutes a permanent rate of 15°5 divisions per minute. It has been said that the experimental adjustment of change of position took only about a minute to effect, yet on renewing the record we find that the effective increase thus esta- blished in the gravitation- stimulus was immediately perceived and responded to by the organ, in an accelerated rate of curva- ture. The permanent in- creased rate at 135° is thus found to be nearly four mesethat at 45°. The organ was now returned to its position at 45°, and the rate once more fell till it became nearly equal to ae Fic. 208. Response Records showing Dif- what it had originally been ferences in Rate of Curvature according ° ; ve as Specimen is held at Angles of 45° ex J “TV at 45, being only very ad pa slightly greater. Theorgan was once more placed at 135°, and the rate again rose, till it reached slightly beyond its former value at that angle. The ratio of the second rates determined at 45° and 135° was also found to be almost as I is to 4 (fig. 208). With reference to the cause of this difference of gravita- tional effect, Haberlandt suggested that it may lie in the fact that the weight of the statoliths in 45° position is on the basal half, while at 135° it is on the apical half (fig. 209) The next point is, to account for the greater reaction of the 502 PLANT RESPONSE apical half. Some light may, perhaps, be thrown on this subject from the results of my experiments on Azophytum. I found that on subjecting this plant to the favourable tonic condition of rise of tem- perature, the younger leaflets began to show spontaneous excitatory response much earlier than the older leaflets. This shows that in an excitable tissue the younger portions are, Fic. 209. Diagrammatic Representation of generally speaking, the Different Positions of a Single Cell, accord- ee : ing as the Specimen is held at an Angle of more sensitive. This 45° or 135°, showing Consequent Redistribu- will probably account tion of Statoliths (after F. Darwin) d A, apical, B, basal, ends of cell. for the difference se geotropic reaction in the case under consideration, for the apical halves of the cells are relatively younger than the basal halves. Determination of the true character of apogeotropic response.—I shall now proceed with the crucial determi- nation of the true nature of gravitational response. In the diagrammatic representation of the multicellular organ, we have the upper and lower responding layers of cells repre- sented by E and E’ (fig. 204). Each of these may be one or more layers in thickness. Since in apogeotropic organs the curvature is upwards, this response may be due (1) to the relative expansion or acceleration of growth of the /ower side, or (2) to the relative contraction or retardation of growth of the upper side. As regards the first hypothesis, the curva- ture induced in grass haulms has been assumed, as stated before, to have proved that the gravitational response is one of accelerated growth. On the other hand, the curvature may be due to the active contraction of the upper side, the response then being of the same nature as was seen demon- strated by the irritating pressure of the magnetic particles. The crucial experiment in deciding between the two alter- i) { ' ' { ! ' , RESPONSIVE CURVATURES—NEGATIVE GEOTROPISM 503 native hypotheses will lie in determining which factor is actively concerned in the production of responsive curvature. If the lower should prove to be the side actively concerned, then the response will be one of expansion ; the activity of the upper, on the other hand, would demonstrate the contractile nature of the response. The principle of the mode of investigation which I adopted in order to decide this question will be understood, if we remember that motile response can be abolished temporarily by application of cold. Thus, if we cool the pulvinus of Mimosa, it ceases to exhibit any responsive contraction under stimulation. Now, in a horizontally placed organ, if the continued responsive curvature be due to the excitatory contraction of the upper side, then local application of cold on that side ought to arrest it. The application of cold on the lower side, however, should produce little effect. But if, on the other hand, the Icwer side should be actively con- cerned in the production of response, then the application of cold on that side would have the effect of arresting the curvature, while its application on the upper would have little or no effect. In connection with this, it should be remembered that a twitch may sometimes be produced by the transient excitation due to sudden variation of tem- perature, but the effect will be short-lived. The permanent arrest of hitherto continuous responsive movement due to sravitational stimulus will only take place when the active side has its power of response abolished by cold. An experiment carried out in this manner would thus decide the question as to whether it is the upper or the lower side that is actively concerned, and also the question as to whether the gravitational response, like all other forms of response to direct external stimulus, is, or is not, funda- mentally one of contraction. In carrying out experiments on the principle described above, I first took a record of the responsive curvature of a Crinum Lily, which was lying horizontally. When a uniform rate of upward movement had been attained, the tip of the 504 PLANT RESPONSE bud moving at a rate of ‘13 mm. per minute, ice-cold water was applied on the upper surface, by means of a strip of cloth, at the point marked in the figure by a downward arrow (1) (fig. 210). In consequence of this the movement is seen to be retarded, and in the course of five minutes it came almost to a stop. The cloth was now removed, at the Fic. 210. Effect on Apogeotropic Movement of Application of Ice-cold Water to Upper and Lower Surfaces alternately of a Horizontally|laid Crinum Lily The first part of the curve shows normal up movement, which is arrested in consequence of the application of cold on the upper surface, at the moment marked with a downward arrow ({). On removal of the application at point marked { | °, the normal apogeotropic movement is renewed, and continues unaffected by the application of cold below at the moment marked with an upward arrow (*). moment marked in the curve, and the movement of response to gravitation recommenced and tended cradually to attain its original value, with the return of the upper surface to the normal temperature. Ice-cold water was next applied on the lower surface, at the moment marked by an upward arrow (1), and it will be seen that this produced no percep- tible effect on the rate of responsive curvature. A similar RESPONSIVE CURVATURES—NEGATIVE GEOTROPISM 505 experiment was performed with a long flower-scape of Urichs Lily laid horizontally. The attachment to the re- cording lever was made with the upper end of the specimen, the lower end being held in a clamp. The specimen was 30 cm. long, and the responsive movement was found to be ‘23 mm. per minute (fig. 211). Here, too, ice-cold water was applied to the upper and lower surfaces alternately, four times in succession, and it will be seen from the figure that an application of cold to the upper surface caused arrest of Fic. 211. Effect on Apogeotropic Movement of Temporary Applications of Cold alternately to Upper (J) and Lower (*) Surfaces of Horizontally laid Scape of Uriclis Lily Application above is seen to produce arrest of movement, while application below has no perceptible effect. the responsive movement, while a similar application below produced no effect that could be detected. These experiments conclusively prove that the funda- mental responsive effect induced by stimulus of gravitation is not acceleration, but contraction, or retardation of growth, precisely similar to the action of other forms of stimulus. Though, under cooling, there cannot be any exhibition of mechanical response to gravitation, yet it appears that the effect of gravitation may be held latent in the organ, as will 506 PLANT RESPONSE be seen from the following experiment. I took three long scapes of Uriclis Lily and laid them horizontally, packed in ice. As long as they were in the ice there was no responsive curvature. After an hour they were taken out of the ice and held erect, and by the time that they were restored to the temperature of the room it was found that the top of the scape in each case was bent from 1°3 to 1°5 cm. in the direction of what had been the upper surface when they were horizontal. This was evidently due to the fact that the cold brought about an arrest of growth; but the geotropic stimulus remained latent, to express itself in a responsive movement later, when growth was renewed, under the action of a favourable temperature. Responsive curvature of acellular organs.-—There is one point, in connection with the induction of gravitational curvature, which might at first sight appear anomalous. In the case of the negative geotropic curvatures of multicellular organs, the fact that it is the upper side which is relatively effective, and that the curvature is the result of its responsive contraction and concavity, is evident from the experiments already described on the local application of cold. When we take an acellular apogeotropic organ, however, suchas the stalk of the sporangium of Mucor, we find that it is the irritated lower side, differentially acted on by weight, whether of sap or statoliths, that becomes convex. This looks at first sight as if the effect of irritation were, as it is generally supposed to be, to induce acceleration of growth, and attendant convexity. We must in this case, however, bear in mind the position of the surface on which the stimulus acts. In our experi- ment on the irritation produced by the pressure of magnetic particles it was the owts¢de surface of the organ that was acted on by stimulus, and it was that side that became concave. In the case of the organ with a single row of cells as described, however, it is the z¢ermal surface which is so irritated, and if we take as our object of observation that internal surface, we shall find that, as in other cases, so here RESPONSIVE CURVATURES—NEGATIVE GEOTROPISM 507 also, the response is by contraction and concavity of the excited surface. The convexity of the outer is thus to be taken as the inevitable result of the concavity of the inner. The fact that in the acellular organ response actually takes place by the concavity to stimulus of the surface acted upon, is further seen in the response of such organs to stimulus of light. Inthe case of geotropic stimulus it is the zzternal surface of the lower side of the organ which is irritated by the differential weight of the cell-contents, and becomes con- cave to the stimulus thus acting upon it. In thecase of light, on the other hand, stimulus acts from the outside, and it is thus the outer or external surface, say, of the same side, which becomes concave. Thus stimulus, acting on the same side in one case from within, and in the other from without, induces responsive curvatures in opposite directions.' ' The explanation of the responsive curvatures of acellular organs has hitherto offered many difficulties. In a multicellular organ, acted on unilaterally by stimu- lus, there is a difference induced in hydrostatic pressure as between the two opposite sides. The diminished turgidity of the proximal, and increased turgidity of the distal, explains the induced curvature. In an acellular organ, however, there cannot be this difference of hydrostatic pressure on the two opposite sides. But the considerations which I shall now offer may perhaps be found to meet the difficulties of the case. The fundamental effect of stimulus is, as we know, to induce protoplasmic contraction, Hence unilateral stimulation acting on an acellular organ may be expected to induce contraction and concavity of the proximal side of the ectoplasmic layer, the result of which will be a curving over of the organ. Asa result of this, the ectoplasmic layer of the distal side will be subjected to tension, which is, as we know, an influence that accelerates growth. Hence the retardation of growth on the proximal, due to contraction, and its acceleration on the distal under increased tension, will combine to produce growth- curvature. A problem of somewhat greater complexity arises in the case of stimulus of light traversing a transparent acellular organ. Let us suppose such a vertical organ to be acted upon horizontally by rays of light from the right-hand side, We have in this case to consider the separate effects of stimulus of light on four different surfaces : (1) the outer ectoplasmic layer of the proximal side Po; (2) the inner ectoplasmic layer of the proximal side Pz; (3) the inner ectoplasmic layer of the distal side Dz ; and (4) the outer ectoplasmic layer of the distal side Do. The contractions of the outer surface of the proximal Po, and the inner surface of the distal Dz, would induce a curvature to the right ; those of the inner surface of the proximal Pz and the outer surface of the distal Do a curvature to the left. But it is evident that as light passes through the organ there must be loss of intensity by absorption. Hence the sum of effective intensities at Po.and 508 PLANT RESPONSE Curvature of grass haulms under gravity.—We shall now take up the consideration of the curvatures induced in crass haulms, laid horizontally, when growth had originally’ been at standstill. It will be remembered that it was the appearance of curvature under such circumstances in the pulvinus of the grass haulm that gave the strongest support to the theory, that negative gravitational curvature in general was due to the increase in the rate of growth on the convex side, rather than to its retardation from active contraction on the concave. In the present case it was argued that since, at the beginning of the experiment, the upper side of the pulvinus was not undergoing growth, it was clear that growth there could not be retarded. The curvature, therefore, must be due to the induction of growth on the convex side, under the stimulation of gravity. This misconception has arisen from the supposition that all curvatures must be induced by differential growth. I have shown, however, (1) that contraction takes place in a stationary organ in response to stimulus; (2) that the unilateral stimulation of such an organ induces concavity ; and (3) that retardation of growth in a growing organ is itself the result of the contractile effect of stimulus. Now, in a horizontally laid grass haulm in which growth has ceased, the upper side—which we have found to be relatively the more effective—will contract under stimulus of gravity That this is the case is seen from the fact that this upper surface is found to become actually shorter than it was before. But, as regards the convexity of the lower surface, the water expelled from the actively contracting upper side will reach the lower, and the increased turgidity thus produced is sufficient, as we have already seen, to re- initiate growth in a dormant tissue. This explains the renewed growth and convexity of the lower side. Thus the curvature of the grass haulm cannot be held to support the at Dz will be greater than the corresponding sum of effects at PZ and Do. As the result of this difference we shall have a right-handed or positive heliotropic curvature, RESPONSIVE CURVATURES—NEGATIVE GEOTROPISM 509 view that the fundamental action of gravitational stimulus is to increase the rate of growth ; it shows, on the contrary, that contraction under stimulation is the active factor. Growth of grass haulms ona klinostat.—I shall here adduce certain considerations which may further serve to explain the difference between the stationary or feebly grow- ing grass haulms, and other normally growing organs, as regards the effects induced in them by the rotation on the klinostat. It is found that in grass haulms, when subjected to the rotation of the klinostat, growth is recommenced, or increased ; while in other normally growing plants there is no such increase of rectilinear growth. For an explanation of this difference we have to recall the effect on growth of increased internal hydrostatic pressure, with the consequent increase of turgidity, which has already been described (p. 428). It was there shown that, when the plant was grow- ing at a moderate rate, the curve of relation between increase of turgidity and increase of growth was practically a straight line ; that is to say, any increase or diminution of turgidity would then produce a proportionate increase or decrease of gsrowth. But this relation did not hold good when the natural rate of growth was feeble or absent. In the latter cases, increase of pressure induced an effect that was dis- proportionately large. And this was specially the case when the growth of an organ had come to a temporary condition of standstill. In that case, when the pressure was gradually increased, growth was found at a certain point to be abruptly renewed, and to go on increasing with the increase of pres- sure, at a rate disproportionately large. If, now, the pressure be once more brought down to what it was just before the point was reached at which growth was started, we find that erowth is not arrested, but persists. Thus the net result of these alternations of pressure is a positive resultant srowth. We have, thus, two distinct cases: (1) that in which the normal rate of growth is moderate, and in which alternate increase and diminution of pressure, acting for equal lengths of time, will induce equal increase and decrease 510 PLANT RESPONSE of growth alternately, the total growth during the whole of the time being the same as it would have been without alter- nation; and (2) that in which, the original rate of growth having been feeble or absent, equal alternations of pressure have the net effect of causing a positive increase of rectilinear growth. Now, in an ordinary growing plant, rotating on the klinostat, we have an instance of the first of these two cases ; for if at any given moment the side A be below, and the side B above, then B under stimulus of gravity will undergo contraction ; hence there will be a diminution of local tur- gidity, and the expelled water will produce an increase of turgidity on the opposite side A. At the end of a semi- revolution of the klinostat, however, this state of things will be exactly reversed, and the alternating effects being thus equal and opposite, there will be no resultant increase of rectilinear growth ; but in the second case—that is to say, of stationary or feebly growing grass haulms—the resultant effect is not #z/, but a positive increase of rectilinear growth. SUMMARY It has been shown that the effect of gravitational stimulus on an apogeotropic organ is fundamentally the same as that of any other form of stimulus, namely, a responsive contraction. A rational explanation of the mode in which geotropic stimulus acts, is afforded by the radial-pressure theory of Pfeffer and Czapek, or by the statolithic theory of Noll, Haberlandt, and Nemec—the essential element of both lying in the hypothesis that stimulus is caused by means of the weight of the cell-contents acting differentially on the inner wall of the horizontally placed cell. That the unilateral pressure of particles is competent to induce responsive curvature of the organ, has been shown experimentally by pressure resulting from the magnetic attraction of iron particles, RESPONSIVE. CURVATURES—NEGATIVE GEOTROPISM 5II It has generally been supposed that the active factor in apogeotropic curvature was the accelerated rate of growth on the convex side of the organ; but it has here been shown by crucial experiments on the unilateral application of cold, that the active factor is really the responsive contraction and retardation of growth on the concave side. CHAPTER XXXVII THE RESPONSIVE PECULIARITIES OF THE TIPS OF GROWING ORGANS Difference between shoot and root in their response to stimulus of gravity— Difference in character of response between tip and growing region of root— Scope of the investigation—Electrical investigation— Responsive results of : 1. Longitudinal transmission of effect of stimulus from tip; (a2) Moderate uni- lateral stimulation ; (4) effect of stronger unilateral stimulation—2. Direct unilateral stimulation of growing region—Moderately strong. stimulus—3. Transverse transmission of effect of stimulus ; (2) moderate stimulation ; (4) stronger stimulation—Mechanical response inferred from observed electrical response—Tabular statement. WE have seen in the last chapter that the responsive effect of gravitational stimulus in an apogeotropic organ is of the same nature as that of any other form of stimulation. In positively geotropic organs like roots, however, this would seem not to be the case, for here the responsive curvature is in the opposite direction. Thus, a root placed horizontally bends in the direction of gravity, and not away from it. It may be urged that there is some polar difference between shoot and root, on account of which, if. the response of the one be regarded as positive, that of the other must be negative; but I have shown that, so far from this being the case, the response of the root to stimulus is precisely the same as that of all other organs, the shoot included, for all alike under stimulation exhibit contraction (p. 76). We shall first see, then, whether in the root there is any difference, as regards the action of gravity, from, for instance, the stem. We know that the growing stem with regard to the gravitational stimulus is both the perceptive and respond- ing organ; for we may cut and isolate any portion of it, RESPONSE OF TIPS OF SHOOT AND ROOT 513 and it will still show an apogeotropic curvature. But the case is quite different with the root. Here the perceptive organ and the responding organ are, as will be seen in the next chapter, distinct and separate from one another. Difference in character of response between tip and growing region of root.—We shall next turn our attention to the peculiar characteristics of the tip of the root as dis- tinguished from the responding region of growth. Darwin, on applying moderate artificial stimulus. unilaterally to the tip of the root, found that it moved away from the source of stimulus ; whereas Sachs, on applying similar unilateral stimu- lus to the responding growing region directly, found that it moved towards it. Thus we see that two different effects are induced, according as stimulation is applied on the responsive zone itself, or transmitted to it from the distant tip ; and in this fact we may perhaps find a clue to the explanation of the opposite effects produced by stimulus of gravity on positive and negative geotropic organs. No explanation has yet been offered cf the opposite characters of these responsive effects induced by similar stimuli, according as they are applied on the responsive zone or on the tip of the root. It was suggested by Darwin that ‘the tip of the radicle is endowed with diverse kinds of sensitiveness ; and that the tip directs the adjoining growing parts to bend to or from the exciting cause, according to the needs of the plant.’' These diverse kinds of sensitiveness have in his view been acquired by the tip of the root, for the final advantage of the plant. The question, then, which we must investigate is, as to whether the peculiar sensitiveness of the tip has been specially evolved by the burrowing root, or is characteristic of the tips of growing organs in general. Should the latter prove to be the case, we have next to account for this characteristic itself. I have already suggested, as a possible explanation of the difference between the responsive actions in apogeotropic and geotropic organs, that in the one case stimulus acts ' Movements of Plants, pp. 552 and 573. LL 514 PLANT RESPONSE directly, producing a movement /owards, while in the other the effect of stimulus is transmitted from a distance, produc- ing a responsive movement in the opposite direction. As against this assumption, however, we are confronted with the fact, which we shall find later, that in some instances of transmitted stimulus of light, the responding organ bends towards, and not away from, the source of stimulation. _ Scope of the investigation.—We have therefore to determine (1) whether or not the tips of all growing organs behave alike; (2) why the behaviour of the tip is different from that of the responsive zone of growth; and (3) under what circumstances the transmitted effect of stimulus causes an organ to move towards, and under what, to move away from, the source of stimulation, at the same time ascertaining clearly the mechanics of such movements. For the purpose of this investigation I shall first use the electrical method of inquiry, which I have already fully described, since it has. the unique advantage of offering unerring indications under the most difficult experimental conditions ; and shall study by its means the characteristic differences of response as between the tip and growing region of a single organ, in this case the shoot. Electrical investigation.—It will be shown that growth- curvature is produced by unequal variations of turgidity on the two sides of the responding organ. This variation can be detected with great certainty by electrical means. I have already explained how a positive turgidity-variation gives a concomitant electrical variation of galvanometric positivity ; and that the true excitatory effect of negative turgidity- variation gives rise to a concomitant electrical change of galvanometric negativity. For the present experiments I took specimens of various growing plants—such as Aryo- phyllum, Cucurbita, and others—and the results obtained from all were alike. In order to obtain unmistakable indica- tions of the effect produced in the responding zone, one electrical connection was made at a point in the growing region, A (fig. 212, a), and the other with a point so distant RESPONSE OF TIPS OF SHOOT AND ROOT 515 that the effect of stimulation could not reach it. As parenchymatous tissues offer great resistance to the conduc- tion of stimulus, it is an advantage to make this second contact with a leaf. In the present investigation we have to study the effect of unilateral stimulation of varying intensity and duration, when applied either at the tip T, or at C, near the responding point A, the latter being in the same longi- tudinal line as the excited points T and c. We shall also study the effect of stimulation of A, on the transverse point (2) (6) (c) Fic. 212. Experimental Connections for obtaining Electrical Response due to Direct and Indirect Effects of Stimulation (a) electrical connections for detection of electrical changes at A, caused by indirect effect of stimulus, from excitation of same side of distant tip, T, and direct effect of stimulus from excitation of adjacent point, C ; (2) electrical connections for detection of electrical changes at B, caused by excitation of diametrically opposite point, A ; (c) electrical connec- tions for detection of relative electric variations of diametrically opposite points, A and B, due to excitation of C, adjacent to A. B (fig. 212, 0), and the resultant effects on A and B, when the point C, near A, is stimulated (fig. 212, c). Before proceeding further, it will be well to consider the theoretical conclusions to which we are led from the demon- strations already made of the turgidity-variations caused by stimulation. The tip T consists, as we know, of undifferen- tiated tissue, which is a relatively bad conductor of stimula- tion. Moderate stimulus, then, at T, might be expected to induce local excitatory contraction, and the water thereby expelled would originate a wave of positive turgidity-varia- tion ; this would reach A, a point on its own line, with greater effect than the transversely placed B. The moderate stimula- tion of T would thus produce a positive turgidity-variation LL2 516 PLANT RESPONSE at Aon its own side. This transmitted effect of increased turgidity we have already distinguished as the zzdrect effect of stimulation. But all cells conduct stimulus more or less efficiently, the difference being one only of degree. Hence an indifferently conducting tissue, such as that of the tip, will conduct the true excitatory state only if the stimulation be sufficiently strong and long continued. The transmitted effect of this true excitation may then be expected to produce negative turgidity-variation, and concomitant galvanometric nega- tivity, at A. When the stimulus is applied, however, at or near the responding point A, we may expect to obtain the dzrect effect of stimulation—that is to say, a negative turgidity-variation and the concomitant galvanometric negativity. To sum up, then, it may be expected that moderate unilateral stimulus applied at the tip T will give rise on the same side of the responding region to the indirect effect of stimulation, which is an increase of turgidity exhibited by a positive electrical variation. The direct effect of stimulus, whether immediate or transmitted, always produces a negative turgidity-variation, evidenced by galvanometric negativity. This effect may be obtained either by the local application of moderate, or by the distant application of strong, stimulus, In making the electrical investigation which is now to be described, I employed various forms of stimulation—thermal, mechanical, chemical, and the stimulus of light. Mechanical stimulus may be applied by friction of emery paper, or by means of a pin-prick. Chemical stimulation is applied by touching the point with a brush which has been moistened in hydrochloric or sulphuric acid. Very dilute acid produces moderate, strong acid a more intense, stimulation. The most perfect mode of stimulation is by means of incident light, the intensity of which may be varied at will. The effect ot stimulus of light, however, will be fully described in the chapter on heliotropism. Another form of stimulation which is also very suitable is the thermal. A short piece of platinum RESPONSE OF TIPS OF SHOOT AND ROOT Ry wire, heated by electrical current, is placed in the neighbour- hood of the point to be stimulated. The intensity of stimulus in this case is regulated by varying either the strength of the heating current, or the distance of the heating wire from the point to be excited. All the different forms of stimulation will be shown to produce the same results. Responsive resultsof: 1. Longitudinal transmission of effect of stimulus from tip : (a) J/oderate unilateral sttmula- tton.— Using the thermal mode of stimulation applied at T (fig. 212, a), I obtained positive electric-variation at A, the record of which is given in fig. 213. A similar result was obtained | Recs Uta) (reese ars Gia? Fic. 213. Record of Positive Fic. 214. Record showing Electrical Variation, indicating Galvanometric Positivity Positive Turgidity- Variation subsequently Neutralisea (represented by Down Curve), under Transmission of induced in Growing Region True Excitatory Effect, iby Moderate Stimulation on due to Continuance of same side of Tip. Time- Moderate Stimulation of marks = minutes the Tip with the mechanical stimulation of a pin-prick. The same was again observed on effecting stimulation by dilute acid. (0) Effect of stronger unilateral stimulation.—I next pro- duced a somewhat stronger thermal stimulation by suitably in- creasing the heating current. This gave rise to the electrical indication of the preliminary positive turgidity-variation, as the immediate effect. But the long-continued action of the stimulus caused the transmission of the true excitatory, which, reaching A, caused the neutralisation of the previous effect (fig. 214). With another specimen, I next applied to the tip a still stronger unilateral stimulus. This caused a brief positive electrical and turgidity variation, followed by a reversed response of 518 PLANT RESPONSE galvanometric negativity, thus proving that the strong excitatory effect transmitted to the organ had not only neutralised, but also reversed, the previous effect (fig. 215). 2. Direct unilateral stimulation of growing region: Moderately strong stimulus.—When stimulus is applied near the responding organ, say at C (fig. 212, @), there is always FIG, 215. Record showing Neutralisation Fic. 216. Record showing and Reversal of Electrical Response Negative Electrical Re- at Kesponding Region, under Strong sponse represented by Up Stumulation of Tip Curve, indicating Negative Turgidity - Variation due This indicates that the firs sitive turgidity- : Sat E cates that the first positive turgidity to Direct Stimplagea variation, due to indirect stimulation, is converted into negative turgidity-varia- tion of transmitted true excitation. produced at A a negative electrical, indicating a negative turgidity, variation (fig. 216). Thus we have obtained, using the same moderate stimulus, two opposite effects, of positive and negative turgidity-varia- tions, according as the point of application was at the distant tip or in the vicinity of the responding organ. It is evident, moreover, that there is a continuity between these two extreme effects, for, as we gradually shift the point of applica- tion from the distant point nearer to the responding organ we observe corresponding intermediate changes, from pure positive, through the neutral, due to equal positive and negative effects, to pure negative. RESPONSE OF TIPS OF SHOOT AND ROOT 519 3. Transverse transmission of effect of stimulus: (a) Moderate stemulation.—We have seen that the direct appli- cation of stimulus at a point has the local effect of a nega- tive turgidity-variation. It is of great theoretical importance that the effect of this stimulus on the diametrically opposite point should be clearly demonstrated. As conductivity has already been shown to be very feeble across a tissue (p. 250), we might expect that it would be the indirect effect, that is to say the positive turgidity-variation, which on stimulation of A would reach the diametrically opposite, or distal, point np. The experiment is carried out, by making galvanometric con- nections with B and L, and applying stimulus at A (fig. 212, 3). On now applying moderate sti- mulus at A, we obtain a positive elec- trical response, indicating positive turgidity-variation, at B (fig. 217)— a result precisely the same as was ,,, ;. 217. Record showing obtained by stimulating the distant Positive ee : ne : tion indicating Positive tip. The effect, then, of stimulating Turgidity-Variation of Dis- any point is to induce a negative tal Point, under Moderate Stimulation of Proximal turgidity-variation of the point itself, and a positive turgidity-variation of the diametrically opposite point. (6) Stronger stimulation—When stronger stimulus is applied, however, at A, the true excitatory effect is gradually transmitted across the tissue, and we obtain at B the neu- tralisation of the preliminary positive effect, as in fig. 214. And, lastly, when the point A is very strongly stimulated, the responsive effect on the diametrically opposite point B is a transient positive, followed by a strong negative variation, as in fig. 215. We have thus studied the separate effects produced at A and B by stimulation of a point near A. The differential 520 PLANT RESPONSE effect as between A and B can be inferred by the algebraical summation of these separate effects. Or it can be directly obtained by making electrical connections with the dia- metrically opposite points A and B, as in fig. 212, ¢c, and applying gradually increasing stimulus at C near A. It will be remembered that these opposite effects of direct and indirect stimulation have received independent demonstration, in both mechanical and growth responses. In Biophytum and in Artocarpus the positive turgidity- variation was proved to be the indirect effect of stimulation, by the erectile responses of the motile organs (pp. 24, 420). In the case of growth-response, again, the indirect effect of stimulation, with the concomitant positive turgidity-variation, was shown in the increased rate of growth (p. 430). The effect of direct stimulation in inducing a negative turgidity- variation, again, was exhibited in Bzophytum and Artocarpus, by the responsive depression of the motile organs. In the case of growth-response, it was exhibited by contraction and concomitant retardation of growth. Mechanical response inferred from observed electrical response.—From the results of the electrical investigation on these turgidity-variations, induced by direct and indirect effects of stimulus, as just described, we are led to conclude that : 1. (a) Moderate unilateral stimulation of the tip gives rise by longitudinal transmission to the zzdzrect effect of stimulus, namely a positive turgidity-variation, on the same side of the distant responding-organ. This will give rise to acceleration of growth and convexity of that side, and by the consequent responsive movement the tip will be carried away from the source of stimulation. (6) When the unilateral stimulation of the tip is a little stronger, the indirect effect of stimulus is neutralised by the subsequently transmitted true excitatory effect, and there is no resultant action. Very strong unilateral stimulation of the tip gives rise by longitudinal transmission to the direct effect of stimulus, namely a negative turgidity-variation, on the same side of RESPONSE OF TIPS OF SHOOT AND ROOT 521 the distant responding organ. This will give rise to retarda- tion of growth and consequent concavity of that side, the tip being carried, by the responsive movement, fowards the source of stimulation. 2. (a) Stimulation of a point at or near the responding region of growth will induce a negative turgidity-variation as the direct effect of stimulus, and at the diametrically opposite point a positive turgidity-variation as the indirect effect of stimulus. This will give rise to concavity of the proximal with convexity of the distal sides. The mechanical effects of negative turgidity- Fic. 218. The Relative Elec- trical and Turgidity Variations variation on one side and _ posi- tive turgidity-variation on the of two Diametrically Opposite Points, A and 8, when Strong and Long-continued Stimulus is applied near a (cf. fig. 2125). other side are thus additive. In this way, the concavity of the proximal and convexity of the distal conspire to bring about the resultant curvature. (0) When the stimulus applied at A is stronger, the effect is a negative turgidity-variation at A; and later, owing to the transmission of stimulus across the tissue, the positive is succeeded by a negative turgidity-variation of B. The resultant effect obtained by algebraical summation thus tends to become zero. When the stimulus at A, however, is still stronger and of longer continuance, the negative response of A will be found to undergo a gradual diminution owing to fatigue, while the transmitted effect at the diametrically Opposite point undergoes an increase. Under these circum- stances the responsive negative change at B will predominate over that at A. The final result may thus be a relative negativity of B, that is to say an effect opposite to that seen We see here (1), in the up curve, the negative variation of the proximal, followed by (2) neutralisation, followed by (3) reversal, that is to say, rela- tive negativity of B. Note the multiple response which here makes its appearance. §22 PLANT RESPONSE in case (a). These different phases of the effect induced by long-continued application of strong stimulus at A—namely the relative negativity of A, followed by neutralisation, followed by reversal or relative negativity of B—are well seen in the record given in fig. 218. Translated into terms of the resulting mechanical response, this would mean: (1) a movement of the organ towards the stimulus ; (2) neutralisa- tion of this movement; and (3) a pronounced movement away. I give here a tabular statement which shows at a glance the various electrical effects and the corresponding mechanical responses which are theoretically to be inferred from them, the experimental verification of these inferences being given in the next chapter. TABULAR STATEMENT OF ELECTRICAL EFFECTS AND INFERRED MECHANICAL RESPONSES Electrical and turgidity effects : : : Mechanical response in the responding region Stimulation theoretically inferred Positive electrical and Convexity of a, and | positive turgidity vari- movement of tip away ation of same side A. from stimulus. | I. Unilateral, of tip, on side A: | (a) Moderate stimulus. | Positive and subsequent No resultant effect. negative effects neu- | tralise each other. | (6) Stronger stimulus. (c) Very strong and long- continued stimulus. by strong negative elec- trical and _ turgidity variations, of same side, A. | Positive twitch, followed | Transient twitch away from, followed by strong movement to- wards, stimulus. | 2. Direct action of | | stimulus on respond- | ing region : | (a) Moderate stimulus. | | | (4) Stronger stimulus. (c) Very strong and long- continued stimulus. Negative variation of side acted upon, and posi- tive variation of dia- metrically opposite side. Positive and subsequent negative effects neu- tralise each other. Positive twitch, followed | by strong negative electrical and turgidity variations of the op- posite side, B. Concavity of the excited, with convexity of the opposite side. Responsive movement of organ towards source of stimulus. No resultant effect. mechanical Resultant response opposite to that in 2 (a); ie. movement away from the stimulus. os pars ere | RESPONSE OF TIPS OF SHOOT AND ROOT 523 The verification of these theoretical inferences will be given in full in the following chapter. SUMMARY From electrical investigation we find that the responsive peculiarities of the tip of the root are not characteristic of that organ alone, but of the tip of the shoot also. The different effect between stimulation applied to the tip and to the growing region of an organ lies in the fact that from the former—it not being a good conductor of excitation —only the indirect effect, that is to say the positive turgidity- variation, is transmitted to the responding organ. In the latter case, however, direct excitation gives rise to negative turgidity-variation. Moderate unilateral stimulation of the tip, therefore, gives rise on the same side of the responsive region to increased turgidity, which, translated into mechanical response, means a negative curvature or movement away from stimulus. Stronger and long-continued stimulation of the tip causes transmission of the direct effect. This will gradually neu- tralise or even reverse the first effect, the positive turgidity- variation giving place to negative. These events translated into terms of mechanical response mean a change from negative to positive response, or movement towards stimulus. Direct unilateral stimulation of the responsive region causes negative turgidity-variation of the proximal and positive turgidity-variation of the distal. The mechanical expression of these will be a movement towards stimulus. By strong and long-continued action the stimulus ceases to be unilateral and becomes internally diffused. The excita- tion thus reaches the distal side. The difference of turgidity- variation on the proximal and distal is thus gradually abolished, or even reversed. The corresponding mechanical response will be a neutralisation or reversal into negative, that is to say a movement away from the stimulus. CHAPTERC-XXXV IM INQUIRY INTO THE LAWS OF RESPONSIVE GROWTH- CURVATURES Scope of the investigation: 1. Mechanical response to unilateral stimulation of the tips of shoot and root: (a) Moderate stimulus—(/) Stronger stimulation— 2. Effect of unilateral stimulus, applied at the responding growing region : (a) Moderate stimulus—(4) Strong or long-continued stimulus—Experiments on the direct and indirect effects of stimulus on A/zmosa: (a) Direct stimula- tion—(4) Indirect stimulation, longitudinal transmission—(c) Indirect stimula- tion, transverse transmission—The curious response of an Avozd—Table showing responsive effects common to pulvini, pulvinoids, and growing organs—Laws of responsive growth-curvature. WE were able, at the end of the last chapter, to pass in review the theoretical conclusions to which we had been led by the electrical mode of investigation, as to the responsive movements which might be expected to follow on the unilateral application of stimulus to the tip and the growing region respectively. I shall now proceed to submit these theoretical conclusions to experimental verification, by taking records of the mechanical movements actually induced. It will be understood here that my object is (1) to show that the peculiar response given by the tip of the root is characteristic of the tip of the shoot also; and (2) to demon- strate the effect of unilateral stimulation on the growing region. We have therefore to study the effects which are induced at the growing region by the action of stimuli of different intensities, according as they are applied unilaterally at the distant tip, or, locally, on the growing region itself. As a specimen of the shoot-tip, we may employ either the tip of a stem or that of an unopened flower-bud. This latter, composed mainly as it is of indifferently conducting elements, LAWS OF RESPONSIVE GROWTH-CURVATURES 525 has all the characteristics of the tip of the organ, while the peduncle below often represents the area of quickest growth, and functions as the responding region. Another advantage of the unopened flower-bud, again, lies in the fact that its upper end is not covered over with appendages like that of the stem. The unopened buds, with peduncles, of Cvocus, then, will be found suitable for this investigation. As specimens of the roots, again, the long straight water-roots of Bindweed are very suitable for these experiments. The records of the responsive movements are taken by means of the recording microscope fully described in a subsequent chapter. The quiescence of the organ, before the application of stimulus, is tested by the fact that the record is a horizontal line. The occurrence of an up curve in the record represents responsive movement towards, while a down curve means movement away from, the stimulus. In this investigation, as in the preceding, various forms of stimulation have been employed. Mechanical stimulus is applied by friction of emery-paper. The jar produced by this causes a temporary disturbance of the image in the field of view of the microscope; but this soon subsides, and the excitatory movement commences after a short latent period, increasing steadily until the effect of stimulus is exhausted. The chemical form of stimulation has the advantage of producing no mechanical jar. The stimulation produced by light, which is the most perfect, will be described in the subsequent chapter on heliotropism. Thermal stimulation is effected by holding a platinum wire, heated by the electrical current, in more or less proximity to the point in the tissue which is to be excited. 1. Mechanical response to unilateral stimulation of the tips of shoot and root: (a) Moderate stimulus.—Applying a single mechanical stimulus of emery-paper friction, of mode- rate intensity, unilaterally to the bud of Crocus, a movement was induced in the responding organ, which carried the tip away from the source of stimulation. This movement persisted for four minutes, after the application of this single 526 PLANT RESPONSE stimulus. I performed a similar experiment on the tip of the root of Bindweed, the response being precisely the same. The rate of movement was in this case somewhat slower, but persisted for eight minutes. The application unilaterally of dilute sulphuric acid brought about exactly similar results in bothcases. The unilateral application of thermal stimulus of moderate intensity, again, to the tip, induced movement away from the source of stimulation in both shoot and root, as will be seen from the record given below (fig. 219). (6) Stronger stimulation,— A somewhat stronger stimulation of the same character caused a movernent away, followed by movement towards, SE ee ee ee Fic. 219. Mechanical Re- In sponses of Shoot, s, and of Root, R, to Unilateral Stimulus applied at the Tip this and following records the down curve indicates negative movement or away from source of stimulus. The up curve indicates positive or movement to- wards source of stimulus. The time-marks represent minutes (cf. fig, 213). the source of stimulus, the resultant effect being neutral. *We now pass on to the effects induced by still stronger stimulation. I have already explained how, when very strong stimulus is applied unilaterally at the tip of an organ, its first and transitory effect is to induce a positive turgidity- variation on the same side of the srowing region. From this we in- ferred the occurrence of a convexity on that side, which would carry the tip away from the source of stimulus. The direct effect of stimulus next reaches the responding region, reversing the first effects and causing a negative turgidity- variation, which would, farz passu, induce a concavity, and carry the tip towards the source of stimulation. In carrying out the experimental verification of these mechanical movements on a bud of Cvocus, I found that an application of strong sulphuric acid on one side of the bud caused it to move first away from, and then very energetically towards, the direction in which the application was made. I next tried to determine the effect of a strong unilateral application of thermal stimulus on the root-tip of Bindweed, LAWS OF RESPONSIVE GROWTH-CURVATURES 527 Here, too, after a transient movement away, there was an energetic movement towards the stimulating heated wire (fig. 220). 2, Effect of unilateral stimulus applied at the respond- ing growing region.—I shall now show that when stimulus is applied near the growing region, it induces effects which are opposite to those resulting from stimulation of the tip. aa Fic, 220. Mechanical Response Fic. 221. Mechanical Re- of Root of Bindweed to very sponses of Peduncle of strong Unilateral Stimulation Crocus, S, and Root of applied at the Tip Bindweed, R, to Unilate- ral Thermal Stimulation This causes a preliminary negative 2 : ; P eee é at the Growing Region followed by a positive, move- ment, that is to say towards the The responses are positive source of stimulus (cf. fig. 215). and towards the source of stimulus (cf. fig. 216). (a) Moderate stimulus—When moderate stimulation of any kind is applied unilaterally in the growing region, the consequent negative turgidity-variation of the side directly excited makes it concave; and a positive turgidity-variation due to the indirect effect of stimulation occurs at the distal side, making that side convex. Thus the induced concavity of the proximal, and convexity of the distal, both conspire to cause a movement of the organ towards the source of stimula- tion. This is seen in the following records obtained with the peduncle of Crocus and the root of Bindweed, the stimulus used having been thermal (fig. 221). 528 PLANT RESPONSE (6) Strong or long-continued stimulus——I have already explained that with moderate stimulation the negative turgidity-variation and concomitant contraction of the proxi- mal, and the positive turgidity-variation and concomitant expansion of the distal, conspire to induce a positive respon- sive curvature ; but when the stimulus is strong or long- continued the excitation is conducted across the tissue to the distal side, which, now contracting, antagonises and reverses the action of the proximal. We have seen this ex- emplified in the electrical response given in fig. 218, where the first positive electrical response of the distal, indicating positive turgidity-variation, was afterwards neutralised and converted into negative by the transverse conduction of excitation. In the case of responsive growth-curvature we obtain results precisely similar. Long-continued unilateral stimula- tion is here often found to neutralise the first or normal effect. Or if, again, the unilateral stimulation be very strong, the proximal side is liable to become fatigued, and the response of the distal to transmitted stimulus being thus predominant, a responsive movement occurs which is reversed or negative, that is to say, away from the source of stimula- tion. Examples of this will be seen in Chapter XLII. I shall also presently give a demonstration of similar preliminary effects with subsequent transverse conduction, giving rise to reversed effects, in the case of the motile response of J7zmosa. We have thus seen that growth-curvature is induced by unequal variations of turgidity, on the diametrically opposite sides of the growing region. We have seen that the effect of indirect stimulation is a positive turgidity-variation. When a feebly conducting tissue is unilaterally subjected to moderate stimulation, the direct excitatory effect cannot be transmitted far, and it is the indirect effect which reaches the responding region, R (fig. 222), and induces convexity. Hence we obtain the typical examples of this effect by stimulating the tip, T, of cither root or shoot. The sensibility of these regions is itself in no way different from that of any other portion of LAWS OF RESPONSIVE GROWTH-CURVATURES 529 the plant-tissue, for they respond to direct external stimula- tion by contraction. But their power of transmitting stimulus is relatively feeble. For this reason, under ordinary circum- stances, they transmit only the indirect effect of stimulus, and it is only when the unilateral stimulus is very strong that the direct excitatory effect is transmitted, inducing the opposite to the usual result, in the responsive concavity of the same side of the growing region. The fact that the sensitiveness of the tip is not fundamentally different from that of the growing region may be demonstrated by applying stimulus to a given point D in the. growing region, and observing the responsive effect induced at Rk, diametrically opposite. The power of the tissue to lic. 222. Diagram showing the various Responsive Effects induced at the conduct stimulus transversely being feeples the result is in this case the same as in that of the ordinary longitudinal transmission from the tip; that is to say, it is the indirect effect that reaches the diametrically Growing Region, R When moderate stimulus is applied unilaterally at the tip, T, or distant point, L, it is the indirect effect that reaches R, and produces convexity. The same convexity of R is induced by stimulation of the transverse point, D; but here the induced curvature is aided by the concavity of the directly excited D. opposite point, R, and induces con- vexity there, this effect being aided, as it happens, by the concavity of the proximal side, due to the direct effect of stimulation. Here again, as before, a stronger or long-continued stimulus may later transmit the direct effect, and neutralise or reverse this first responsive curvature. A third case arises when unilateral stimulus is applied at L, lower down on the stem, at some distance from the respond- ing region, and if this be sufficiently feeble, it will be the indirect effect which will reach the same side of the respond- ing region, and produce convexity there. Stronger or long- continued stimulation will in this case, as before, neutralise or reverse the first effect. MM 530 PLANT RESPONSE I have been at some pains to make these examples of the direct and indirect effects of stimulus clear; for all the complex curvatures of growth, which appear at first sight so anomalous, are ultimately resolvable into these. And since the subject is so important, I shall add still another demon- stration, which is capable of easy repetition, and will be found to be striking and convincing. Experiments on the direct and indirect effects of Stimulus on Mimosa.-—We have now seen that all growth- curvatures may be analysed into, (1) that contraction, with consequent concavity, which is concomitant to the negative turgidity-variation that consti- tutes the direct, or transmitted direct, effect of stimulus ; and (2) that expansion, with consequent convexity, that is concomitant to the positive turgidity-variation, which constitutes the indirect FG. 223. Experimental Arrange- effect of stimulus. Now, the ment for obtaining Records f 2 on Smoked Drum of Responses negative turgidity-variation is ane oe ee be Leaf of exhibited in the case of the Mimosa motile leaf of J7z7osa by depres- Thermal stimulator at Ss produces sion, and the positive turgidity- direct stimulation, and conse- Boe By quent fall of leaf. Moderate variation by erection. stimulation, at a distant point, I shall now explain the S,,, gives rise to indirect effect : of erection. experimental arrangements by which the plant itself may be made to record these opposite effects. The indicating leaf is attached to the short arm of a long writing lever. This lever consists of the quill of a long tail-feather of a peacock. Its short arm, 1 cm. in length, is tied by a thread to the petiole of the leaf. A fine needle is passed through the quill, and rests on frictionless supports which may be glass tubes. The longer arm of the lever, 10 cm. in length, has a piece of bent aluminium, with a sharp point, tied to the end, LAWS OF RESPONSIVE GROWTH-CURVATURES 531 to serve asa writer. This writing-point is, by the elasticity of the feather, pressed lightly against the smoked-paper surface of a vertical revolving drum (fig. 223). When the leaf is under no stimulation, either direct or transmitted, the record is a horizontal line. But true excita- tion produces a depression of the leaf, causing an up curve. The erectile response, on the other hand, which is due to indirect stimulation, produces a down curve. The records given in the following figures are accurate reproductions of some which were taken in this way (fig. 224). (a) Daurect Stimulus is applied by the close proximity of a V-shaped plati- num wire, heated electrically. Its intensity is varied by varying either the distance of the stimu- Fic. 224. Mechanical Responses lating wire or the intensity of the a a wei stimulation. — (a) record of responsive fall when heating current. When stimula- tion is now applied directly at s, that is to say, near the respond- ing organ, response takes place by a negative turgidity-varia- tion, producing a fall of the leaf. This is seen in the up curve (fig. 224, a). (0) Indirect stimulation, lon- gitudinal transmission. — Sti- mulus of moderate intensity is stimulus applied near the re- sponding organ (cf. fig. 221) ; (4) response when stimulus is applied on same side, but at greater distance, s,. A pre- liminary erectile response is here followed by the true excitatory depression. This is due to the indirect effect first transmitted being succeeded by the direct. Had the stimulus applied been feebler, or more distant, there would have been only the first, or indirect erectile effect, similar to fig. 225 (cf. fig. 220). now applied lower down on the same side of the stem, at S,- This is observed first to induce the positive turgidity- variation, causing erectile response, which is due to the indirect effect of stimulus, and is shown in the preliminary down twitch of the curve (fig. 224, 6). Later, the direct effect is trans- MM2 532 PLANT RESPONSE mitted, causing the fall of the leaf, as shown in the up curve. When the stimulus is feebler, or applied at a still greater distance, the indirect effect alone reaches the organ, and only the erectile response, due to. posi- tive turgidity-variation, results, being similar to that shown in the next record (fig. 225). (c) Indirect stimulation, transverse transmisston.—We next obtain the very interesting case in which feeble stimulus is applied at the transverse points, The record (fig. 225) shows that we have here an erectile response due to the positive turgidity-variation mia.” Gere ae tee of indirect stimulation. But if this sponse of Leaf of Mimosa ttansverse stimulus be made strong due to Transmission of or be long continued, the direct effect Indirect Effect to Distal . : Side, when Proximal, s,, 1S transmitted somewhat later, and, is Stimulated in that case, we obtain a fall of the Mt simulus qucte “stronger, Jeaf, preceded by the positive erectile by the fall of the leaf twitch, which is similar to that shown due to the later trans- : : : ae 2 mission of true excitation. in the Biot Aes be record (fig. 224, 6). The response would then The curious response of an become like that of fig. Ari ve . -plai 224 (6) (cf. fig. 219). risema.—This fact will explain a very remarkable phenomenon which I have noticed in certain species of Arisema, that grow on the mountains round Darjeeling, at a height of about 7,000 feet. This plant, before flowering, consists of a long petiole bearing a terminal whorl of leaflets, which are arranged like rays in a strictly horizontal plane. Later, however, the inflorescence, borne on a peduncle enclosed within its spathe, breaks out from one side of this petiole. Unilateral mechanical stimu- lation is thus undoubtedly brought about, and gives rise to indirect stimulation on the distal side, which, as we have just seen, causes an erectile mechanical response. In the case of this Avisema, it isa striking fact that immediately after flower- ing, the most distal leaflet of the whorl—that is to say, the LAWS OF RESPONSIVE GROWTH-CURVATURES 533 leaflet which is situated in the diametrically opposite line— hitherto horizontal, becomes abruptly vertical (fig. 226). This is the only leaflet which stands out from its fellows, and it is invariably found to be situated on the line diametri- cally opposite to the flower, such differentiation being induced only after flowering. All the variations exhibited by diverse forms of response —electrical, mechanical, responsive acceleration or retarda- tion of growth, and growth-curvatures—are only so many Fic. 226. Curious Response of Ariseema Before flowering the leaflets lie in a horizontal plane, as seen in the figure to the left ; but when inflorescence breaks through unilaterally, the indirect stimulation of the distal side causes erection of the diametrically opposite leaflet, as seen in figure to the right. expressions of these two fundamental phenomena, the effects of direct and indirect stimulation. It is these two variables which, conjoined with stimulus unilateral or diffuse, give us all those manifold effects that at first sight would appear to belong to different classes of phenomena. That such a unity does actually underlie them all may be seen ata glance from the following concise statement of responsive effects, induced in pulvini, pulvinoids, and in growing regions which act as pulvinoids. 534 PLANT RESPONSE TABLE SHOWING RESPONSIVE EFFECTS COMMON TO PULVINI, PULVINOIDS, AND GROWING ORGANS Stimulation diffuse Stimulation unilateral Effect on proximal side Effect on distal side I. DIRECT EFFECT. 3. DIRECT EFFECT. 4. CORRESPONDING | EFFECT. Negative turgidity- Negative turgidity- Positive _ turgidity- variation. variation. variation. Galvanometric nega- Galvanometric nega- Galvanometric posi- * tivity. tivity. tivity. Depression of motile Contraction and Expansion and con- leaf. | concavity. ! vexity. : Retardation of ca a ees ere growth. When stimulus is strong or long continued, the true excitatory effect passes to the distal side, neutralising or reversing the first response. | 5. TRANSMITTED 6. CORRESPONDING | DIRECT EFFECT. EFFECT. Negative turgidity- | Positive _ turgidity- variation. ' variation. ! : . eth Galvanometric nega- Galvanometric posi- | tivity. tivity. | Contraction and Expansion and con- concavity, in re- vexity in respon- sponsive region. | sive region. | 2. INDIRECT EFFECT. 7. INDIRECT EFFECT. | 8. CORRESPONDING EFFECT. Positive _ turgidity- Positive _ turgidity- | Negligible. variation. variation. Galvanometric posi- Galvanometric posi- | tivity. tivity. Erection of motile Expansion and con- | leaf. vexity in respon- Acceleration of sive region. growth. | REMARKS.—It will be remembered that the indirect effect is a secondary con- sequence of the direct contractile effect of stimulus on the excited point. Thus the motive power is the active contraction of that point. The indirect effect, described as No. 7, is exemplified by the moderate unilateral stimulation of the tip of shoot or root. When this stimulus is stronger, or sufficiently long continued, we havea transmission of the @7ec¢ effect of stimulus, and case No. 7 | is displaced by case No. 5. | ! The induction of concavity, of either upper or lower side of the pulvinus, by local stimulation will be found demonstrated in a subsequent chapter. Confining our attention to the effects induced at the srowing region, we arrive at the following laws of growth- curvature, LAWS OF RESPONSIVE GROWTH-CURVATURES 535 Laws of responsive growth-curvature.—It must be remembered here that the effect of indirect stimulation is to cause an increase in the rate of growth, and that of direct stimulation a retardation of the rate. By a fosztive effect is meant a responsive mechanical movement towards, and by negative, away from, the source of stimulation. I. Unilateral stimulus of moderate inteusity, applied at the tip of root or shoot, gives rise to a negative effect, the tip being moved away. 2. Stronger or long-continued stimulus, applied unilaterally at the tip of root or shoot—being conducted gradually to the growing region—-results in a neutralisation of the first negative by a subsequent positive effect. Or there may be a resultant positive, due to the predominance of the transmitted effect. 3. Direct unilateral stimulation of moderate intensity on the growing region causes a positive response or movement towards stimulus. 4. Strong or long-continued unilateral stimulation of the growing region, on account of the transmission of effect to the distal side, may give rise either to neutralisation of the normal, or to a reversed or negative effect, that is to say, to movement away from stimulus. SUMMARY All the mechanical’ effects induced at the responsive growing region of either root or shoot, by unilateral stimula- tion, may be summarised as follows, it being understood that positive response means movement towards, and negative, movement away from, stimulus : (1) Posttzve response is induced, first, by direct unilateral application of stimulus on the growing region ; second, by the long-continued unilateral application of moderately strong stimulus at the tip. (2) Negative response is induced, first, by the unilateral application of feeble stimulus at the tip ; and second, by long- continued unilateral application of strong stimulus at the srowing region, causing fatigue of the proximal, and trans- mission of true excitation to the distal, side. CHAPTER XXXIX INQUIRY INTO POSITIVE GEOTROPISM No specific difference as regards their responses between shoot and root— Darwinian curvature—Localisation of geotropic sensibility at the root-tip— Experiments as to whether amputation of root-tip abolishes excitability—The tip of the root the organ of gravi-perception—The perceptive versus the responding organ—True perceptive region. WE have in the course of the last chapter studied in detail the expression in growth-curvatures of the direct and indirect effects of stimulus, and have found them to be, under known conditions, very definite. We have seen that the responsive effects produced at the tips of root and shoot are in no way different from those which have been observed in other parts of the tissue. The specific sensitiveness hitherto so generally attributed to the root-tip is thus found not to be justified. The differences of effect which have been noticed, according as tip or responding region is the point of stimulation, we have seen to be due merely to the transmission of the indirect instead of the direct effect. I shall next proceed to show that the opposite curvatures induced in shoot and root by stimulus of gravitation are really due, not to two distinct sensibilities positive and negative, but to the action of a single stimulus which acts in one case directly, and in the other indirectly. Darwinian curvature.—It has thus been shown that the responsive curvature manifested by the radicle on stimulation of its tip is not characteristic of roots alone, but, under similar circumstances, of the shoot also. It has not, then, been specially evolved for the advantage of the plant. As proving this, the abrupt conclusion of certain experiments of my own has a peculiar interest. The tip of the root INQUIRY INTO POSITIVE GEOTROPISM 537 was in these cases subjected to strong unilateral stimulation by the proximity of a heated platinum wire. Instead of responding, however, by movement away, as would have been the case had the advantage of the plant been the primary object of its action, the root-tip gave only a preliminary spasmodic twitch in the negative direction, and then reversed its movement ; the organ now turned towards the heating wire, fell upon it, and was burnt.!_ There is thus no protective adaptation here, any more than in the case of the moth which is impelled to throw itself upon the destroy- ing flame. No choice exists for either of these, for, in both alike, the movement is due to the working of the inexorable laws which govern the phenomenon of response. In the case of the plant, that increased turgidity which is the in- direct effect of a distant stimulus always induces an increased rate of growth ; but when the stimulus is sufficiently strong or long continued, its direct or true excitatory effect, being trans- mitted, brings about retardation of growth. And each of the diverse curvatures of growth, induced by the unilateral application of stimulus, forms a particular case of the work- ing out of these two laws, of the direct and indirect effects of stimulation. Having now arrived at certain definite conclusions as _ to the mechanism of the responsive curvature induced by unilateral stimulation of the tip of root or shoot, it will be well to pass in rapid review.the exhaustive series of experi- ments on the responsive behaviour of the tip of the radicle, which were carried out by Darwin, more especially as some of the cases which he noted as somewhat exceptional will be found, in view of the investigation which I have described, to be susceptible of very simple explanation. He pro- duced unilateral stimulation in three different ways, first, by attaching minute fragments of cardboard to one side of the ‘ The same thing happens, in a still more striking manner, when the wire is placed in front of the growing region, and suddenly héated. The excitatory effect on the proximal side is then so great that the organ at once rushes upon the stimulating wire and is scorched. This occurs before the distal side could be excited by the transmitted effect of stimulus, 538 PLANT RESPONSE root-tip by means of gum or shellac varnish. The moderate and constant irritation which was produced in this way was usually found to induce a convexity on the same side of the growing region. His second method was chemical. He touched one side of the tip with silver nitrate. This also, generally speaking, induced a convexity similar to the last. The third and last method of unilateral stimulation employed by Darwin was a slanting cut, resulting, in the majority of cases, in the same responsive curvature as before. All these cases, it will be understood, are illustrations of the indirect effect of moderate stimulation. But we have seen—in the second law of responsive growth-curvature—that under the long-continued action of a stimulus sufficiently strong, this first, or negative, effect is neutralised by the subsequent conduction of excitation (p. 52). The degree of such conduction and the power of neutralisa- tion will obviously depend on the conducting power of the particular specimen. Now, it was found by Darwin, in several of his experiments, that the negative effect was followed by neutralisation. This he regarded as somewhat exceptional. We have seen, however, that such a result, under certain circumstances, was to be expected. We have also seen, in the same law of responsive growth- curvature, that when the transmitted stimulus is strong, it induces a reversed or positive curvature. And in some of Darwin’s experiments also, especially those in which he used the strong stimulation of caustic, this was observed. Finally, in some six cases, Darwin found that on applying unilateral stimulation to the tip there followed an induced concavity. With regard to this it need only be said that the sensibility of the tip to direct stimulation is not specifically different from that of any other tissue, All tissues contract, in response to direct stimulation. The difference between the contractile powers of the tip and the growing region is one merely of degree, that of the latter being relatively greater, and for certain reasons, to be described later, the more evident. Visible contraction INQUIRY INTO POSITIVE GEOTROPISM 539 of the tip can only be seen under considerable stimulation, or when that part of the tissue is highly excitable. Localisation of geotropic sensibility at the root-tip.— I next turn to Darwin’s very important demonstration of geotropic perception as residing in the root-tip. He showed this by extensive researches on the fact observed by Ciesielski, that on the decapitation of the root-tip the geotropic action is found to disappear. Various objections have been urged against this view, which I shall be able to show to be groundless. Sachs, for instance, has argued that if such sensibility resided in the root-tip, there is no reason why the tip of the shoot should not exhibit the same. This argument is not, however, valid, inasmuch as the statolithic or other elements, which by their weight cause stimulation, may be concentrated locally in the root-tip, and more diffusely distributed in the shoot. And that the general sensibility of the root-tip to other forms of stimulation is not different from that of the shoot-tip I have already fully demonstrated. Francis Darwin has shown, again, that localised gravi- perceptive areas may occur in organs other than the root. In the seedling of Sorghum, for example, he finds this sensi- bility to reside in the cotyledons. The second objection that has been raised is that the shock of amputation might either abolish the general ex- citability or arrest the growth of the root, on which the responsive growth-curvature depends. With regard to the abolition of excitability, it is true that the effect of a strong stimulus, such as that of cut, would persist for some time, yet there is always recovery, after a longer or shorter interval. I have tested the persistence of the fatigue caused by amputation, by the electrical method, and I find that with certain plants, such as Aryophyllum, the recovery from the effect of amputation-shock is very rapid, taking place in the course of about a minute. The longest persistence of the after-effect which I have been able to observe was in the case of Colocasia, where it lasted for a period of 540 PLANT RESPONSE from forty-five to sixty minutes. It is thus unlikely that amputation would permanently abolish the power of renewed response. With regard, next, to the contention that amputation would be liable to arrest that growth of the root on which responsive curvature is supposed to depend, it is to be borne in mind that though growth-activity is a sign of excitability, yet we may have excitability without growth. Hence it is quite possible that responsive curvature might be initiated in a tissue which was not beforehand in a con- dition of growth by the stimulus of gravity, as indeed was found to be the case with grass haulms. Investigation on supposed abolition of excitability by amputation of root-tip——The question as to whether am- putation of the tip abolishes the general excitability of the growing region of the root, can only be decided finally by the direct observation of the variation, if any, in the mechani- cal response of the root to stimulus after amputation. Should there be no such variation, then —the researches of Ciesielski and Darwin having proved that a root deprived of its tip does not respond to gravity—it will follow that the tip of the root alone is geotropically sensitive. That this is so—that is to say, that amputation does not permanently modify the general sensibility of the root—I shall next proceed to demonstrate. The experiments that have been carried out by different observers, for the purpose of determining whether amputation of the root-tip produces any modification of the rate of growth, have led to very various results. Some have found that it has the effect of inducing an accelerated rate of growth ; others, again, state that it induces no change whatever in the rate; and still others find that it causes retardation of growth. All these discrepancies are reconciled, however, when we remember, as I have demonstrated, that indirect stimulus causes increase, and direct stimulation retardation, of growth. Moderate stimulation, therefore, by section of the extreme tip, giving rise to the transmitted effect of indirect INQUIRY INTO POSITIVE GEOTROPISM 541 stimulus, would accelerate growth; while a section made in a better-conducting tissue, or nearer the growing region, with its stronger stimulation, would transmit the direct effect, with concomitant retardation of growth. But a stimulation of intermediate intensity would initiate no change. I shall, however, describe an investigation by which the question of the variation of responsive excitability in the growing region by amputation is put to the test of direct experiment. For this purpose I mounted a straight water- root of Bindweed in my Crescograph, and took a record of its normal rate of growth. It should be stated that the root was attached to the Optic Lever at a distance of 3 mm. below the tip, which was thus left free for amputation. The normal rate of .growth was considerable, being 0105 mm. per minute. I next took a FIG. 227. Curves showing Effect of Amputation on Rate of Growth record of three successive con- tractile responses to thermal shocks (fig. 227). After this, without disturbing the ar- rangement, the root-tip was amputated, to a distance of and Response in Root of Bzrdwzed (z) continuous line indicates normal rate of growth. Dotted curves represent three responses to three successive uniform stimuli; (4) exhibits the rate of growth and responses after amputation. It will be seen that there is practically very little variation. 2mm., and a record of the growth was again taken after an interval of fifteen minutes. It was now found to be practically the same as before amputa- tion, the rate being ‘0100 mm. per minute. The contractile responses to thermal shocks of the same intensity as before were also, to all intents and purposes, the same. With another specimen the rate of growth before section was ‘0066 mm. per minute ; after amputation of 3 mm. of tip, the rate was 0055 mm. The mechanical responses before and after section were almost the same. These experiments, then, show that amputation of the root-tip does not abolish the general ex- citability of the responding region. The fact that the gravita- tional response of the root, however, is abolished by the removal 542 PLANT RESPONSE of the tip, proves that, as pointed out by Darwin, the tip is, with regard to gravity, the perceptive organ of the root. The tip of the root the organ of gravi-perception.—The inference that it is the root-tip which is the perceptive organ has also been arrived at by Pfeffer and Czapek, pursuing an independent method of investigation which discarded amputation. There is again the very suggestive fact that the starch grains whose weight, according to the statolithic theory, brings about stimulation, occur generally at the root-tips alone. Thus three different lines of research lead to the conclusion that it is in that region that the gravi-perceptive power resides, the power of response by curvature being behind, in the region of growth. The perceptive versus the responding organ.—In view of much existing vagueness on this subject, it is desirable here to obtain clear and well-defined conceptions as to how far the differences commonly insisted on between perceptive and responding organs are justified. To take the familiar instance of Mzmosa, we know that when stimulus is applied, for example, on the stem, there is no perceptible motile effect in the stem itself, but excitation is markedly manifested at the pulvinus. From this it has been erroneously supposed that the stem merely perceived, and did not respond to, stimulus. It has, however, been shown in previous chapters that every part of the plant-tissue responds to stimulus by contraction, the visible and striking manifestation seen at the pulvinus being simply the result of certain accessory ana- tomical facilities which happen to exist at that point. Thus, every part of the plant-tissue is both perceptive and responsive to external stimulus, to a greater or less extent. When we come to the growing organ, responsive curvature is induced by the concavity of the excited side. And as the zone of growth is, generally speaking, the region of the greatest excitability, the greatest cur- vature usually takes place there, that region functioning INQUIRY INTO POSITIVE GEOTROPISM 543 in this respect as a diffuse pulvinoid. It should be re- membered, however, that the excitability of a tissue does not disappear on the cessation of growth, and responsive curvatures are thus sometimes seen to extend even beyond that zone. Contraction takes place, as we have seen, in all tissues, but it is relatively much less in the very young and the very old. The responsive contraction at the tip is therefore slight and com- paratively inconspicuous. Indeed, it can only be made visible under very strong stimulation. That contraction is induced, however, is shown in the fact that a wave of positive turgidity- variation reaches the zone of growth by excitation of the tip. This can only be brought about by the expulsion of water, which is the result of excitatory contraction at that point. Under direct stimulation, then, every tissue, whether at the tip or in the growing region, undergoes a similar contraction. The sensitiveness, or sign of response, of all tissues is therefore the same ; but about these signs we are liable to be deceived if we pay attention to the conspicuous effects alone, for the local application of a unilateral stimulus at the tip gives rise to little perceptible concavity, while its transmitted indirect effect, lower down in the growing region, induces a relatively great convexity. Moreover, the whole length of the organ above the point of greatest convexity acts as a kind of lever- index, causing high magnification of the responsive movement. The tip, on the other hand, possesses no such magnifying index. These considerations will clearly show that there is no difference in kind between the sensitiveness of the tip and that of the growing region, both alike having the power of perceiving and responding, in different degrees, to external stimulation in general. Just as the tip itself merges physically into the growing region, so these respective responses pass imperceptibly into one another. Though moderate unilateral stimulus of the root-tip causes the characteristic movement away from stimulus, yet strong stimulation induces movement towards it. Stimulus applied on other parts of the plant also will induce movements similar to that caused by mode- 544 PLANT RESPONSE rate stimulation of the tip, provided the effect transmitted to the growing region be indirect. True perceptive region.—There is a sense, however, in which it is true that the root-tip alone is the perceptive region, but this statement applies exclusively to the stimula- tion caused by gravity; for, in order that this may take place, the presence of statolithic or other elements through which it can act is necessary. But if such elements be concentrated at the root-tip, it is clear that that region alone can become the seat of stimulation. This is not the case with other forms of stimulus, which act directly, the pre- sence or absence of statoliths being a matter of indifference. This distinction is important, since the outward resemblance of effects at the root-tip, in the two cases of stimulation by light and stimulation by gravity, has sometimes led to the assumption that statolithic bodies formed the medium of excitation in both. That such, however, is not the case is now evident. In the geotropic root, then, it is the indirect effect of moderate stimulus, unilaterally transmitted from the distant tip, that causes the responsive curvature. In the case of the apogeotropic stem, on the other hand, it is the direct stimulation of the statolithic elements distributed throughout that region that induces the responsive curvature; and since I have shown that the curvature caused by direct is opposite to that which is the result of indirect stimulation, it will be seen that the opposite curvatures observed in root and shoot are not due to different sensibilities possessed by the two organs. It is to be noted, moreover, that although the statolithic or radial pressure theory affords a very rational explanation of the mode in which stimulation is brought about by gravity, yet the explanation which I have offered, of the occurrence of opposite geotropic effects in root and shoot, does not in any way depend upon the ultimate validity of this or any other particular theory. The aim of my demon- stration has been to show that, through whatever means the stimulus of gravity may act, it is inevitable—from the fact INQUIRY INTO POSITIVE GEOTROPISM 545 that the stimulation of the shoot is direct, and that of the root indirect—that a single identical stimulus should in the two cases induce opposite responsive effects. Thus, though for the sake of convenience it is necessary to distinguish the upward curvature of the stem from the downward curvature of the root, by such terms as apogeo- tropic and geotropic, yet these words must not be allowed to connote two different sensibilities to the action of gravity ; for the sensibility of irritable tissues to stimulus is always of one kind, and of one kind only. SUMMARY The responses induced by stimulation of the tips of shoot and root are similar under similar circumstances. The amputation of the tip of the root does not, normally speaking, abolish the excitability of the growing region of the root. The abolition of geotropic action in the root, after amputation of the tip, shows therefore, as pointed out by Ciesielski and Darwin, that with regard to gravity it is the tip that is the perceptive organ. This conclusion is also supported by the experiments of Pfeffer and Czapek, in which amputation was not included. The statolithic particles, again, through whose weight stimulation by gravity is pro- bably brought about, are, generally speaking, found con- centrated at the root-tip. Direct stimulation has been shown to induce a responsive movement in one direction, and indirect stimulation in the opposite. The opposite geotropic curvatures in apogeotropic and geotropic organs are therefore due, not to the possession of different sensibilities, but to the fact that in the former stimulation is direct, and in the latter indirect. CHAPTER XL ON CHEMO-TROPISM AND GALVANO-TROPISM General difficulties of the investigation—How to overcome these difficulties— Three distinct methods of testing results: (1) by variation of longitudinal growth ; (2) by responsive movement of pulvinus ; and (3) by growth-curva- ture—Method of application of chemical reagent—Effect of alkali—Effect of acid—Effect of copper sulphate—Action of sugar solution—Chemo-tactic movements—-Explanation of anomalous osmotic or plasmolytic action—Excita- tory versus plasmolytic reaction in pulvinus of JA/mosa: (1) Favourable tonic condition—(2) Ordinary tonic condition—Polar effects of currents inducing growth-curvatures—Localised polar effects on pulvinus—Anodic and kathodic effects on longitudinal growth—Generalised law of polar excitation in plants— Galvano-tropic response—The indirect effect of polar excitation—The effect on growth of ‘ electrification ’ of soil. Iv will be understood that growth-curvatures take place so slowly, that the effects of various external agents in inducing them can only be observed visually after the lapse of con- siderable intervals of time, which may be a matter of hours, or even of days. This fact is sufficient of itself to introduce elements of complication into the problem; for (1) the plant may in that time undergo unknown spontaneous variations ; (2) the fundamental effect of any given stimulus is subject, when too long continued, to reversal, as we have seen in the case of the leaf of MWzmosa, which from normal contraction passes into fatigue-relaxation, under the long- continued action of stimulus; (3) the subjection of a plant during too long a period to an abnormal condition may sometimes cause derangement of its general functions ; and (4) there is the further element of variation which depends upon the point of application of stimulus itself, since we have seen that stimulation, acting directly on the responding organ, may produce one effect, and indirectly exactly the opposite. It CHEMO-TROPISM AND GALVANO-TROPISM 547 has doubtless been due to the action of these sources of varia- tion that the observations made by investigators have so often been contradictory. All these uncertainties disappear, however, when we employ the high magnification, and the method of continuous record, which have been described ; for by these means we are enabled to follow the different phases of effect which occur immediately on the application of stimulus, and during its continuation. As these curvatures, moreover, are the results of the unilateral application of stimulus, expressed in the one-sided modification of longitudinal growth, the previous demonstration of the effect of the diffuse application of the same agent on growth itself enables us to infer the result to be looked for. The experiment is thus resolved into a verification of the inference. Again, I have shown that the growing region acts like a diffuse pulvinoid. Experiments then, by unilateral applica- tion of the particular stimulus to a true pulvinus, offer us another and independent means of testing the results arrived at. Thus we have no fewer than three distinct methods of testing the action of a given agent in the induction of growth- curvature, which, if they corroborate each other, may be regarded as affording a rigorous demonstration of the results. These are: first, the variation of longitudinal growth under the diffuse action; second, the responsive movement of a pulvinus under the unilateral action ; and, third, the curvature which represents the modification of growth induced by the unilateral action of a given stimulus or agent. Method of application of chemical reagent.—In dealing with problems involving the unilateral application of chemical reagents particularly, the experimental arrangements which [ am about to describe will be found suitable. In order to obviate the complications which might result from the indirect effect of stimulus applied at a distance, the chemical reagent should be applied directly on the growing region. For this purpose the petals of various flowers, while in an active state of growth, are very appropriate, and I have used particularly NN 2 548 PLANT RESPONSE those of the Indian Champaca (Michelia Champaca). The claw of a detached petal is held in a clip, and the petal is immersed in the inverted position in a small glass trough of water, of cubical shape. Inside the trough, and almost touching one face of the petal, say with its own right surface, is a partition of mica, in which is a long narrow slit. Thus the petal is in the left-hand compart- ment of the cube. A microscope of low magnification is focussed on the marginal point of its tip. The required chemical solution is now dropped by means of a pipette into the right-hand compartment. Thus it is diffused through the narrow slit, and acts directly and virtually unilaterally on the proximal side of the petal. If the particular agent should be one which accelerates growth, that is to say induces expansion, a growth-curvature will be induced, the proximal side becoming convex ;' but if its action is to induce depression or retardation of growth, the permanent effect will be a concavity of the proximal. The movements induced are observed by means of the microscope. Effect of alkali.—I\n studying the effect of various drugs on growth in Chapter XX XV. we saw that the characteristic effect of alkali was to produce a contraction and retardation. In the present experiment, on the unilateral application of sodium hydrate in the manner described, the proximal side of the petal was found to become concave, the tip being carried towards the agent. Effect of acid.—The general effect of acid was found, as will be remembered, to be opposite to that of alkalis, namely relaxation. On now applying solution of HC! unilaterally to a petal of Champaka, the result was the convexity of the proximal side. Effect of copper sulphate.—This 1 have found to arrest growth, and its unilateral application in the present case was observed to induce concavity of the proximal side of the petal. ' The sudden introduction of a reagent may occasionally give rise to a transient excitatory contraction, but the effects described here are the permanent growth- effects. CHEMO-TROPISM AND GALVANO-TROPISM 549 Action of sugar solution.—A dilute solution of. this agent has already been shown to accelerate growth. Its unilateral application was found, in the present case, to bring about convexity of the proximal side of the specimen; but very strong solutions of sugar were found, as we have seen, to retard growth, and the unilateral application of such a solu- tion was now found to induce concavity of the proximal side. Chemo-tactic movements.—We have now seen that the excitatory or depressing action of various agents is indicated, in the case of the growing organ, by a curvature one way or the other. Similar movements are induced also in pulvinated organs; but in organs which are capable of multiple re- sponse we shall expect to find that the corresponding effects induced by such agents will consist in similar movements often repeated—that is to say, in the initiation of such multiple response, or in its appropriate modification. It will be shown in Chapter XLIX. that the swimming movements executed by ciliated organisms form an instance of such multiple response. It will further be shown in that chapter that the organs respond to stimulus, whether chemical, photic, thermal, or electrical, by swimming either towards or away from it. Explanation of anomalous osmotic or plasmolytic action.—An attempt has sometimes been made to explain the responsive movements of plant-organs as the result of supposed osmotic variations within the tissue. Thus it was held by De Vries that the growth-curvatures of multi- cellular organs were brought about by an increase of osmotic substances on the convex side, giving rise to augmented hydrostatic pressure. It was afterwards ascertained, how- ever, that the convex side of the curved organ did not con- tain any greater quantity of osmotically active substances than the concave. The method of plasmolysis of De Vries is often used for the determination of the differential osmotic activity and turgescence on the two sides of a curved organ. After plasmolysis, the curve induced in the organ is flatter than before, and from this it has been inferred that the cell- 550 PLANT RESPONSE sap of the convex side of the organ is more powerfully osmotic than that on the concave. But various effects have been observed, of which the occurrence of plasmolysis alone affords no explanation. Thus, the diminution of curvature, which De Vries showed to be a consequence of plasmolysis, was demonstrated by Noll, in the case of recently curved organs, to be only the second phase of the effect; for at the beginning of the operation in these cases, as he pointed out, the curvature of the organ was actually increased. This opposition of effects, the increase of curvature, followed by the flattening of the curve, as the result of plasmolysis, has not hitherto met with any satisfactory explanation. These obscurities in plasmolytic reactions have arisen from the fact that the excitatory action of some of the plasmolysing reagents has not hitherto been recognised. That such an influence may be exercised, and sometimes in opposition to osmosis, was, however, demonstrated in my experiments on suctional response, where it was seen that a strong solution of sodium chloride applied at the roots, instead of arresting suction by the withdrawal of cell-sap, actually enhanced the rate of suction for some time (p. 383). It was also shown in a later chapter, dealing with the effects of chemical reagents on growth, that when favourable tonic conditions had been induced, fairly strong salt solutions, instead of the usual retardation of growth, brought about a temporary enhancement, followed by depression (p. 486). We thus see that the responsive movement is modified (1) by the condition of the tissue, and (2) by the duration of the application of the solution. Now, in an already curved organ, we have an induced anisotropy, or difference of con- dition, on the two opposite sides. The effects of chemical reegents, then, on such an organ will be complex, for they will differ on the two sides, in intensity, and also in phase of reaction. The observed responsive movement in increasing or diminishing the existing curvature will thus represent the algebraical summation of the effects on the two sides. CHEMO-TROPISM AND GALVANO-TROPISM 551 Excitatory versus plasmolytic reaction in pulvinus of Mimosa: (1) Favourable tonic condition.—\ have already stated that a moderately strong solution of sodium chloride is a stimulating reagent. It is therefore to be expected that its diffuse application on the pulvinus will bring about a fall of the leaf ; and this, as we shall presently see, is found to be the case. We saw, however, in studying the effect of sodium chloride on growth-response, that under the favourable tonic condition induced by the optimum temperature of 34° to 35° C. it gave rise to a preliminary relaxation, followed later by contrac- tion. From this it occurred to me, that in the parallel response of the pulvinus of JZzmosa we might expect, under similar favourable conditions, to observe two opposite responsive movements : first, a preliminary ex- pansion, exhibited by erection of the leaf, and afterwards a contraction, exhibited by the depression of the leaf. In carrying out the experiment, I cautiously raised the temperature Fic. 228. Response of j Leaf of JAZimosa in of the pulvinus to 34° C. and then Favourable Tonic Con- dition to Chemical Sti- applied to it a 3 per cent. solution of malig: O64 pee Cenk sodium chloride at the same tem- Salt Solution perature. The responses were auto- = The es nea! faa . 1S lere seen 0 e matically recorded by the leaf on erectile. smoked paper wrapped about the revolving drum. It will be seen (fig. 228) that the preliminary effect was a movement of relaxation or erection, which was completed in the course of one minute, and was followed by the opposite movement, or fall of the leaf. In these two opposite responsive movements, then, occurring in succession to each other, we have an analogous case to that of the two opposite and succeeding modifications of curvature, observed by Noll in a curved organ, immersed in salt solution. 52 PLANT RESPONSE UL (2) Ordinary tonic condition.—It will thus be seen that inferences drawn from experiments on plasmolysis may lead us to very wrong conclusions unless we take full account of the possible excitatory action of the particular solution. This is made strikingly evident by the following experiment. If we apply a strong solution—say IO per cent.—of sodium chloride to the pulvinus of J/zmosa, then, arguing entirely from the theory of plasmolysis, we must expect that the withdrawal of water will cause flaccidity of the tissue, and so bring about the fall of the leaf. This flaccidity, further increasing with the duration of application, would tend also to increase the fall of the leaf progressively. But a similar fall Fic. 229. Response of Leaf of AZ@mosa in Ordinary Tonic Condition to the Chemical Stimulus of 10 per Cent. Solution of Salt Under continuous stimulation the normal responsive fall is followed by fatigue-relaxation. may, on the other hand, be due to an excitatory reaction caused by strong salt, and there is nothing at first sight which would enable us to distinguish the one from the other. We know, however, as regards J/zmosa, that under a con- tinuous application of stimulus—as, for instance, rapidly succeeding mechanical or electrical shocks—the first fall of the leaf is succeeded by a return to the erect posi- tion. If the predominant effect of salt, therefore, in the given instance, be to induce a responsive fall by excitation, then its continuous operation should give rise to a subse- quent erection. And from the automatic curve recorded on the smoked drum this is found to be the case (fig. 229) ; CHEMO-TROPISM AND GALVANO-TROPISM 553 for it demonstrates that the fal] of the leaf of M/zmosa under the action of a strong solution of salt was due, not to plasmolytic, but to excitatory action. Polar effects of currents inducing growth-curvatures.— The galvano-tropic effects noticed by various observers in the case of growing plant-organs have been contradictory, because of the many complicating factors which might be expected to arise, not only from the differences of anodic and kathodic effects, but also from the varying points of application, and intensity and duration of current. All the obscurities and anomalies in connection with this subject will, however, be found to disappear, when they are related to the funda- mental polar effects, which have already been established as applying to the excitation of plant-organs (Chapter XVI.). I have shown that the kathode excites at make, and the anode at break. I shall now be able to show further that the galvano- tropic effects observed are all deducible from these. Inci- dentally, too, owing to certain special advantages afforded by growth-response, we shall be able to discover additional effects which could not have been detected by the mechanical response of the pulvinus alone. We have hitherto investi- gated the polar effects of an electrical current acting on the pulvinus as a whole; but in order to bring these funda- mental effects into the clearest prominence, and to establish their universality, I shall now study (1) the reaction induced in a limited area of the pulvinus ; (2) the variations of longi- tudinal growth which result from the electrical actions of anode and kathode ; and (3) the growth-curvature induced by the unilateral action of anode or kathode on a growing region. Localised polar effects on Pulvinus.—It is usually believed that the responsive effect seen in a pulvinus is entirely due to the excitability of the lower half of the organ. I have already shown that it is really due to the differential excitability of the upper and lower halves, and that the upper half is also contractile under stimulus, though in a less 554 PLANT RESPONSE degree than the lower. This will be seen fully demon- strated in heliotropic response, where the localised action of moderate light on the upper surface will be found to induce a movement upwards. In the present case, a given electrode, carrying current, will be applied locally on the upper half of a pulvinus, the second electrode being applied at a consider- able distance on the stem, and the specific result recorded. For such local application, it will be understood that a large pulvinus is an advantage, and therefore I used for my experiment the pulvinus of Erythrina indica, the sensitive- ness of which is not so marked as that of d/zmosa. As preventing the diffusion of stimulus to the lower side, this lessened sensitiveness of the organ constitutes an added advantage for the purposes of my experiment. The respon- sive effect, it must be remembered, can be magnified to any extent that is desired by the Optic Lever, and in the present investigation a magnification of 200 times was employed. If the effect of the given electrode (laid on the upper sur- face), then, be to induce a responsive contraction, we can see that the normally horizontal leaf will be moved upwards, whereas a responsive expansion or relaxation would bring about a movement downwards. In order, however, to show the essential unity of all the different kinds of responses, whether by mechanical or growth movements, we shall regard them as cases respectively of the two fundamental phenomena of expansion and contraction. Expansion thus causes convexity or acceleration of growth, represented in these records by up curves, while contraction, concavity, or retardation of growth, is shown by down curves. In my experiment on the upper half of the pulvinus of Erythrina, | first made it anode, the E.M.F. used being twenty volts. In previous experiments on the anodic and kathodic reactions of the pulvinus, it was the differential effect that was observed, and the magnification employed for the record was only slight. Hence the pure effect of anode- make was found to be inconspicuous, whereas the anode- break induced the usual marked contractile action. In the CHEMO-TROPISM AND GALVANO-TROPISM 555 present case, however, no complicating differential action being involved, and the magnification being considerable, we are able to detect a make-effect, which is contrary to the break-effect at anode—that is to say, it induces an expan- sion and consequent convexity. This anode-make expan- At break the sion is quick, and soon reaches its maximum. usual contractile effect is induced, but this is not here very clearly dis- tinguishable from the movement of a natural recovery (fig. 230). The current was now reversed, making the upper half of the pulvinus kathode. This induced a contraction and concavity which, unlike the quickly exhausted effect of the Fic, 230. Polar Effects ot anode-make, went on increasing for Currents due to Localised 2 Application on Upper Half some time. At kathode-break we of Pulvinus of Zrythrina ae see not only the cessation of the a 4 ; Up curve, in this and in fig. contractile effect, but probably also Baa Deieoaents “expansion an expansion, aided by the natural and convexity. Down curve represents contrac- process of recovery. Other experi- tion and concavity. Con- by 1 tinuous curve represents ments will be described presently, Hae ae Saree ei which will clearly show that not dotted curve shows the only do anode and kathode. induce eo hres aia opposite effects, but that each at its own make and break exhibits re- actions, which, though not of equal intensity, are of contrary signs. These interesting opposite effects, at make and break respectively, vexity induced at anode- make. Aé= responsive con- cavity at anode-break. Kw = induced concavity at kathode-make. Kd = ex- pansion induced at kathode- break. The time-marks in this and following curves represent minutes. are not easily observed in muscle under ordinary condi- tions, inasmuch as muscle which is in the usual state of expansion cannot be further expanded; but Bieder- mann has found that smooth or cross-striated muscles, which are partially contracted, exhibit local expansion at the anode-make. Again, he found in the case of cardiac muscle that the kathode-break also gave rise to local expansion. 556 PLANT RESPONSE These opposite effects at make and break, which I have already demonstrated in the case of the pulvinar response of vegetable tissue, will next be exhibited in a still more striking manner in the case of growth-response. Anodic and kathodic effects on longitudinal growth.— We thus pass from the question of the motile response of pulvinated organs to the polar effects of currents on the rate of growth. From a demonstration of the fundamental action here, we shall be able to infer the curvature which will be induced by the unilateral application of polar currents in the growing organ. Experiments on growth, especially when conducted by the Method of Balance, have the unique advantage that the opposite responsive effects, of expansion and contraction, are clearly distinguishable, and not liable, under any circumstances, to be confused with the natural process of recovery; for, as I have explained before, when a balanced horizontal record is taken of growth, an expansion or acceleration of the rate of growth will give rise to a deflection, represented, say, by an up curve. Recovery to the normal condition will now be indicated by a horizontal record. Contraction or retardation, similarly, will be represented by a deflection of the record downwards. Using this method, then, I have studied the effects of anode and kathode on the growing regions of both root and shoot, the second electrode being placed at a very great distance from the first, so that its effect may be considered non- existent. It may be said here that the results obtained were the same for both root and shoot, and I shall now describe the effects observed in the case of an experiment on the root of Bindweed, using an E.M.F. of twenty volts. One of the electrodes—a moist strip of cloth—is wrapped about the growing region; the second, as said before, being at a great distance. The balanced horizontal record of growth is first taken, and the growing region is made kathode. From the down curve in the record (fig. 231), it will be seen that a responsive retardation of growth is induced, which goes on increasing for a considerable time during the maintenance CHEMO-TROPISM AND GALVANO-TROPISM 5a of the current. The current was now interrupted, and as an effect of the kathode-break we obtain an expansion and acceleration of growth above the normal, as seen from the up curve. This acceleration persisted for nearly three minutes, after which the growth-rate became again normal. The current was now sent in a reversed direction, and the result of this anode-make was a sudden expansion and acceleration of growth, which, as in the case of motile response, persisted for a relatively short time, when the growth-rate, as seen from the return to the horizontal, became once more normal. The current was now in- terrupted, and as the result of anode- break we have a contraction and retardation of growth, which per- Effects of Anode Fic. 231. and Kathode on Variation sisted for nearly a minute and a half, after which the growth-rate became again normal. From this experiment we again see that not only are the anodic and kathodic effects opposite, but that the effects on each of make and break are also opposite. It is also seen that the expansional effect caused by anode- make is relatively smaller, and of Rate of Growth in Root of Bindweed exhibited by Balanced Growth-record Down curve represents con- traction and retardation of growth, hereand in fig. 233. Up curve represents expan- sion and acceleration of growth. Kz =contraction at kathode-make ; Kd= ex- pansion at kathode-break ; Am=expansion at anode- make, and Aé=contraction at anode-break. It will be noticed in this and other occurs, and is completed, more rapidly than the kathode-make con- traction, whose effect is more per- sistent. From these data we are able to arrive at a more comprehensive law of polar ex- citation in the case of vegetable tissues than was given in Chapter XVI, including not only ordinary mechanical responses, but also the modifications induced in growth. It will be understood that with regard to growth, expan- sion means acceleration of growth, and contraction means retardation. figures that the contraction at kathode-make is stronger and more persistent than the expansion at anode- make. (oe) PLANT RESPONSE UL wt The generalised law of polar excitation in plants is as follows: The kathode induces contraction at make, and expansion at break. The anode induces expansion at make, and contraction at break. Galvano-tropic response.—From the demonstration just made of the fundamental polar effects on growth, we can easily infer the responsive curvature that would be induced in a growing organ by the unilateral action of anode or kathode. I shall, however, show that the effects directly observed are in strict conformity with these deductions. For these experiments I todk the long scape of Crinum Lily, and on its growing region I applied one electrode, the other being connected with a distant point. On now making one side of the growing region anode, there was induced on that side a sudden and short-lived responsive expansion and convexity, seen in the up curve (fig. 232). At Dreakgetiee opposite effect occurred, consisting eee. an Nee partly of contraction and partly of Crinum Lily by Uni- natural recovery. The current was lateral Application of — now yeversed, and the effect of the Anode and Kathode , ; Mo em ee unilateral kathode-make was the induc- make; Ad = contrac: tion of a very active and relatively long- tion at_anode-breaks Continued contraction and concavity, to Kw = contraction at kathode-make, and be seen from the down curve. The a eee od kathode-break next gave rise to the opposite effect, which was made up partly of expansion, and partly of natural recovery. Thus in these galvano-tropic effects of currents on growing organs, we find what is simply a special case of the fundamental polar effects of currents on growth in general. The indirect effect of polar excitation.—For the pur- pose of clearly demonstrating the fundamental action of currents, I have taken in the first place the simplest cases of the direct action of anode and kathode on the growing CHEMO-TROPISM AND GALVANO-TROPISM 559 responding region itself; but when similar applications are made, at a point distant from the responding region, we can foresee the fact that variations of these effects will occur. Thus the kathode-make, whose direct action induces contrac- tion and concavity, will now, acting from a distance, bring about, by means of expelled water, the indirect effect of expansion and convexity. This can be demonstrated by selecting a growing and undetached petal of Champaca, in which the growing region is diffuse. The lower half of this petal is held in a clamp, the free upper half being attached to the Optic Lever for observation of its responsive movement. If now we make one side of the clamped half kathode (the other electrode being connected with the rest of the plant at a distance), then, at make of kathode, the indirect effect of stimulation, reaching the same side of the free end of the petal, gives rise to expansion and convexity—that is to say, the opposite effect to that which would have occurred had that region been directly subjected to kathodic action. The effect of the kathode on a distant growing organ is thus an acceleration of growth. The effect on growth of ‘electrification’ of soil.— Empiric attempts have been made to discover whether the maintenance of an electrical current through the soil might or might not be made to have the effect of accelerating the growth of plants. This problem can only be satisfactorily solved, however, from accurate knowledge of the direct and indirect effects on growth of currents under the given condi- tions. Since growth is determined by the direction of current, it is not clear that currents flowing through the soil from right to left, or left to right, could give rise to a single effect. Looking, again, at the root, on which the current acts, we can see that one side will be anode and the other side kathode, and as the actions of these are known to be antagonistic, it is at first sight difficult to see how there can be any resultant excitatory effect at all. We are, however, enabled to obtain a clear understanding of the subject by an attentive comparison of the responsive 560 PLANT RESPONSE effects of anode and kathode (figs. 230, 231). It is to be borne in mind that the growth of a plant, other factors remaining the same, is dependent on its suctional activity. And this again depends on the excitation of the root. Now, we saw that though the effects of anode and kathode at make are opposite, yet they are at the same time not equal. The anodic effect is relatively small and short-lived, and the kathodic effect is stronger and more persistent. Hence, though the effects on the two halves of the root may be electrically opposed, yet there will be a resultant differential effect of excitation, due to the predominance of kathodic action. Hence an electrical current, whatever be its direction, would excite the roots, causing a greater suctional activity, with consequent enhance- ment of growth. This deduction I have been able to verify by experimenting on the variation of the rate of growth of seedlings of Orysa sativa when the soil Fic. 233. Effect in Acceleration of Rate . é of Growth of Seedling of Oryza sativa in which they grow was of Current through the Soil subjected to a current, The continuous line shows effect during passage of current through the soil flowing now in one, and (1) from left to right (->), and (2) rom again in the opposite right to left (<). Dotted line shows : - effect on cessation of current. Record direction. The growth- of growth taken under balanced con- record of a single seed- eer ling was first taken under balanced conditions (fig. 233). The electrical current was next sent through the soil from left to right, and during the continuation of this current, we see from the up curve the acceleration of growth above the normal that took place. The current was now stopped, and the former induced acceleration was replaced by a brief retardation, as seen in the down curve, after which the normal rate was restored. The current was next reversed, and yet the response was one of accelerated growth, and on the stoppage of the current after a brief retardation there was again a restoration of the normal rate. This experiment CHEMO-TROPISM AND GALVANO-TROPISM 561 was carried out using an E.M.F. of ten volts, the two elec- trodes being applied at a distance of 10 cm. from each other. This meant a potential gradient of one volt per cm. This relatively strong voltage was applied for the sake of obtaining measurable response within a short time. But a much smaller E.M.F. is found to produce similar effects, though the action is slower. The application of a strong E.M.F. has the disadvantage of inducing fatigue, which is a drawback not present in the use of a feeble E.M.F. It is, however, shown that, during the continuation of a current through the soil, the rate of growth of a plant is enhanced. SUMMARY The effects of unilateral chemical and galvanic excita- tion may be studied, both from pulvinar and growth movements. The unilateral application of alkali gives rise to positive curvature, that is to say, a movement towards the stimulat- ing agent. Acids, under similar circumstances, give rise to negative curvature. The unilateral application of copper sulphate gives rise, by retardation of growth, to a concavity or positive responsive movement. The unilateral action of dilute solution of sugar, by enhancing the rate of growth, induces negative response. A strong solution, however, gives the reversed or positive response. Effects fundamentally similar are expressed by swimming organisms in appropriate movements to and from sources of chemical stimulation. The assumption that the variation of curvature which occurs when an already curved organ is placed in strong solutions of salt is due to the action of plasmolysis is not always justified, for such solutions also exert characteristic excitatory effects. The pulvinus of Mzmosa in a favourable tonic condition ome) : e 562 PLANT RESPONSE reacts to the action of salt, at least for a time, by an erectile responsive movement. Under a less favourable tonic con- dition, it responds by depression. That this latter is not entirely due to plasmolytic action is seen from the fact that the leaflet subsequently becomes erected, as is the case under continuous stimulation. The localised and unilateral action of anode and kathode on a pulvinated organ ts as follows : The anode-make causes expansion, inducing convexity. This effect attains its maximum in a short time. The anode-break induces contraction and_concavity. The kathode-make induces contraction and concavity. This effect is stronger and more persistent than the opposite effect of expansion at anode-make. The kathode-break induces expansion and convexity. The polar effects on longitudinal growth are as follows : The anode-make causes expansion and acceleration of growth. This effect attains its maximum in a short time. The anode-break causes contraction and transient retarda- tion of growth. The kathode-make causes contraction and retardation of growth. This effect is stronger and more persistent than the opposite effect of acceleration at anode-make. The kathode-break induces an expansion and transient acceleration of growth. The localised and unilateral action of anode and kathode on a growing organ, results from the unilateral exhibition of those growth-variations which have just been described. Since a growing organ is virtually a diffuse pulvinoid, the effects are exactly similar to those seem in a pulvinated organ, already described. Owing to the fact that kathodic action is relatively stronger than anodic, a feeble or moderate current flowing through the soil exerts an excitatory action on roots, by which the suctional activity of the plant is increased. The result is an increased rate of growth, which is independent of the direction of flow of the current through the soil. PARE Vill HELIOTROPISM 002 és + : CHAPTER Er FUNDAMENTAL RESPONSIVE ACTION OF PLANT-TISSUES TO STIMULUS OF LIGHT Diversity of movements induced by light-—Differentiation of responsive movements —Action of light on tissues in sub-tonic conditions —Effect of light on pulvinated organs—Effect of diffuse stimulation of light on non-growing radial organs— Retarding effect of light on longitudinal growth— Phenomenon of oscillation under long-continued stimulation—Similarity of responsive reaction under light and under other forms of stimulation. THERE is perhaps no other phenomenon in the plant-world which is at once so striking and so universal as that of the response which is evoked from plants by the stimulus of light. Under this influence the plant as a whole, and every part of it, is tremulous. Not only does *the growing stem curve towards or away from the incident rays, but every leaf under their action is thrown into a state of periodic daily rhythm. By the absorption of light, again, the plant is energised. Thus the two factors, of external stimulus and internal energy, whose manifestations are so opposite in character, are brought into play Diversity of movements induced by light. —Light, as a form of external stimulus, evokes responsive movements, which appear to be extremely diverse in their nature. Radial organs, for example, such as stems, under certain conditions, direct themselves towards the light, and under others, again, away from it. Leaves, however, behave quite differently. These are said to possess the peculiarity of placing themselves with their surfaces at right angles to the light; but even this is not a property universally exhibited, for there are certain leaves which place themselves in a direction coinciding with 566 PLANT RESPONSE that of light, either towards or away from it. Some plants, again, close their leaves or leaflets on the approach of night, in the so-called ‘ position of sleep ;’ while a further complica- tion arises from the fact that an apparently similar ‘sleep’ movement is produced, in these or others, by the action of the noonday sun. Light, again, appears in some cases to initiate movement, as was seen in Desmodium at standstill, and in others to arrest it, as is said to happen with the spontaneous movements of Trifolium pratense. Certain swarm-spores, moreover, appear to be attracted by light, swimming towards it, with energetic beats of their cilia ; while others, on the contrary, are affected in the opposite manner, and swim away. Or the same specimen may be found to swim, now towards, and again away from, light, swinging backwards and forwards like a pendulum. We thus see that not a single responsive effect of light has been observed in the case of plant-organs of which an example directly to the contrary may not be found. For this reason it has appeared hopeless to attempt to unify these phenomena, and this fact has left investigators with little option but to tend towards a ‘belief in the individuality of the plant in deciding what shall be the effect on it of external conditions.’ ! So far we have been considering only the diversity of the responsive effects which are induced by light in plants. When we come, however, to the further question of the responsive mechanics by which the stimulus of light evokes these movements, we are confronted at the outset of our inquiry by the fact that, as Pfeffer says, ‘the precise character of the stimulatory action of light has yet to be determined.’ ? Differentiation of responsive movements.—lIt is cus- tomary, in treating of plant physiology, to draw sharp lines of demarcation between the different classes of movements which are to be attributed to the action of light, ascribing each to ' Francis Darwin, B.A. Report. : 2 Pfeffer, Physiology of Plants, English translation, 1903, vol. il. p. 101, RESPONSIVE ACTION OF PLANT-TISSUES TO LIGHT 567 some unknown specific sensibility. Thus, Sachs differentiates some of the principal effects as follows : ‘In the case of that stimulation of light which produces waking and sleeping, the stimulus lies in the varzatzons of the intensity of light ; it is not the light as a constant force which effects these movements, but the varying intensity. A further great difference between the heliotropic curvatures and those which bring about the sleep movements, lies in the fact that the organ can make heliotropic curvatures in all directions. The movement of waking and sleeping, on the contrary, only takes place in one plane, which divides the leaf and motile organ symmetrically, and it is thus unimportant here in what direction the rays of light fall upon the motile organ, but only important that light is present at all, or increases or decreases in intensity. The above will suffice for the distinction of the movement of waking and sleeping from the heliotropic curvatures.’ ! Finally, he summarises the differences of motile effects as follows : ‘We may thus say shortly, the movements of waking and sleeping are called forth by paratonic light stimulus, whereas the spontaneous movements of the same leaves are in- dependent of any light stimuli, but probably dependent on phototonus. Heliotropic curvatures, on the contrary, have nothing to do with phototonus.’ It will be found, however, that all these effects, sharply differentiated as they are, may be seen in one and the same organ. We may take for example the terminal leaflet of Desmodium gyrans. This exhibits under favourable condi- tions autonomous movements, whose period is short. It exhibits also daily periodic movements, with the very long period of twenty-four hours. It further, as I shall show, exhibits positive heliotropic curvature when exposed to one- sided illumination. Of these effects, it is supposed that the autonomous movement is independent of the paratonic action of light, but probably dependent on phototonus. The daily ' Sachs, Phystology of Plants, English translation, p. 628. 568 PLANT RESPONSE periodic movement is held to be dependent on paratonic effects and phototonus.. The heliotropic effect is ascribed to the continuous action of light, independent of phototonus. Thus in the same organ we have to postulate various irritabilities and mechanisms, in order to account for its multifarious. movements. Are there, then, independent or. different irritabilities, coexisting simultaneously in the same organ? Such a state of things is so difficult to imagine, that it prompts us to try to look at the problem in a fresh light, divested of all assumptions which are incapable of experi- mental proof. Approaching the matter thus directly, then, we see that, instead of so many different irritabilities, there may possibly be, fundamentally, but a single phenomenon of irritability, finding expressions apparently diverse, in consequence of the anatomical or physiological differentiations of the responding organ. If this should be so, the question will resolve itself into three separate inquiries. First, what is that responsive action which constitutes the characteristic effect of stimulus of light? Second, is such response to light unique in character, or is it a single instance of that universal phenomenon of contraction which we have seen to be the response of all excitable cells to stimulus in general? And, lastly, in what manner do the various anatomical and physio- logical differentiations of responding organs operate to modify the expression of this response ? Action of light on tissues in sub-tonic condition.—Before proceeding to a decisive demonstration of the nature of the effect of light on excitable tissues in a normal condition, however, I shall briefly refer to the suggestion which has been offered, that light in some unknown way induces a lessening of turgor, which brings about diminution of growth. This theory could not hitherto find acceptance for want of a precise knowledge of the exact nature of the stimulatory action of light, and of the relative significance of absorbed energy in promoting growth. Against the assumption that light diminished turgor, it was urged that the pileus of RESPONSIVE ACTION OF PLANT-TISSUES TO LIGHT 569 Coprinus drooped in darkness, and became turgid on restora- tion to light ; and the further supposition that light dimi- nished growth was held to be negatived by the instance of a dark-rigored plant, in which growth, so far from being retarded, was accelerated or renewed by simple exposure to light. ’ In both these instances we notice the abnormal condition induced in the plant. We must bear in mind that that rhythmic activity which is essential to growth depends, like all other rhythmic activities, not only on the turgor of the tissue, but also on the energy which it has absorbed. We have seen that when the sum total of independent stimulating factors present in the plant is adequate to raise its tonic condi- tion above par, then rhythmic activity is initiated or renewed. Among these factors are, as has been shown: (a) a proper condition of turgidity ; (4) favourable temperature ; and (c) that previous absorption of energy of light which determines what we may distinguish as the phototonic condition. The fact that, besides turgor, a certain amount of energy is also necessary to initiate growth has been fully demon- strated in a previous chapter. Taking now the case of the dark-rigored plant, we see that the arrest of its growth is due to a deficit of absorbed energy—in this instance, phototonus,. Under these conditions, the renewed exposure to light would be sufficient, by supplying the missing factor, to re-initiate rhythmic activity and consequent growth. The drooping of the pileus of Coprznmus in darkness is another expression of the sub-tonic condition of the plant. It must be remembered that the suctional activity which determines turgor is itself dependent on the rhythmic activity, and therefore on the tonic condition, of the plant. The critical element of this tonic condition may in certain cases be the absorption of light. I have noticed a similar drooping in flowering plants kept in the dark for a longtime. Exposure to light, restoring the tonic condition, is in such cases, as also in Coprinus, sufficient to restore the normal turgidity of the plant. In connection with this, I may draw attention to the fact 570 PLANT RESPONSE that the after-effect of absorbed stimulus in maintaining a favourable tonic condition is more prolonged in some cases than in others. In Bzophytum, for example, the rhythmic activity by which the leaflets are thrown into pulsation is maintained only as long as the stimulating factors are acting. In Desmodium, on the other hand, there is a considerable capacity for storage of energy, and rhythmic activity persists for a long time, even on the removal of external stimulating agencies. : We thus see that the question of turgidity-variation alone is not sufficient to explain the action of light on a plant. We have also to take into account the important element of energy. We have briefly considered the effect of light on tissues in an abnormal condition; but our main inquiry concerns itself with the precise nature of the stimulatory effect of light on tissues which are in a normal tonic condition. I propose to demonstrate the character of this action by three independent lines of investigation. First, we shall study the action of light on pulvinated organs, in order to see whether or not this stimulus produces the same kind of differential contractile effect as other forms of stimulation. Secondly, discarding those complications which inevitably result from the differentiated structure of the pulvinated organ, I shall proceed to determine the precise nature of the fundamental form-change undergone by a tissue when excited by light. For this purpose I shall subject a radial organ to diffuse stimulation of light, observing whether or not under these circumstances it exhibits longitudinal contraction. And, lastly, I shall study the effect of light in inducing changes in the rate of growth, and shall also try to find out whether the fundamental responsive action discovered in the case of stationary, that is to say of non-growing, organs, might not be capable of explaining the observed variations of srowth under the action of light. | Effect of light on pulvinated organs.—If a strong beam of light be applied to the pulvinus of the leaf of Mzmosa RESPONSIVE ACTION OF PLANT-TISSUES TO LIGHT 571 or to the pulvini of the leaflets of Bzophytum, it is known that a responsive movement, similar to that evoked by any other form of stimulus, is induced. Now, we have seen that the response of such pulvini is given by means of differential longitudinal contraction. If, then, the effect of stimulus of light in this case is to produce a differential longitudinal contraction, we ought to be able to obtain from a radial organ, subjected to stimulus of light from all sides, a respon- sive longitudinal contraction. Effect of diffuse stimulation of light on non-growing radial organs.—In order to demonstrate this, I took a radial style of Datura, and subjected it to the stimulus of light from all sides, by throwing a beam of sunlight which struck it on one side directly, and on others by reflection from properly inclined mirrors. On exposing the specimen to this stimulus for a period of four minutes, a responsive contraction of seven divisions was induced. On the cessation of light, there was recovery in the further course of a period of nine minutes. | next applied stimulus of light for six minutes, and a respon- sive contraction of eleven divisions was then induced, with a subsequent recovery on the stoppage of light, which was completed in fifteen minutes. We thus see that the effect of stimulus of light in producing responsive contraction is precisely the same as that which is the result of any other form of stimulation ; that a feeble or short-lived stimulus induces a corresponding effect, from which recovery takes place, in a comparatively short time; and that a strong stimulus—unless it induce fatigue—will bring about a considerable contractile effect, from which the recovery is accomplished in a proportionately longer time. Again, I find that continuous stimulation of light produces a maximum tetanic effect, and that too long-continued action, bringing about fatigue, may induce fatigue-reversal —contraction passing into relaxation—as we found in the case of J/zmosa, and of various radial organs which were subjected to too long continued action of stimulus, 572 PLANT RESPONSE Retarding effect of light on longitudinal growth.—_-We thus see that the fundamental contractile effect of stimulus is precisely the same in the case of light as in that of any other form of stimulation. We know also that the response of a growing organ is the same as that of one which is not growing. It has been shown further, that in a growing organ the contraction due to direct action of stimulus had the effect of retarding growth. Now, from the fact that the effect of light is the same as that of other forms of stimu- lation, it follows that the result of its direct action on a growing organ should be to produce contraction, and i te ! i n i Be ee RA IS —- L 10" 15’ 20’ 25° 30\35’ 40° 45’ 50° 55° 60’ 65’ 70° Fic. 234. Longitudinal Contraction and Retardation of Growth under Light in Hypocotyl of Szapzs nigra The first part of the curve shows the normal rate of growth. Arrow (|) indicates moment of application of diffuse light, which is seen not only to retard growth, but also to induce a marked contraction. The second arrow indicates moment of withdrawal of light, and dotted portion of the curve shows recovery. resultant retardation of growth. Though this conclusion, however, was thus clearly established in theory and by other experiments, using different forms of stimulus, I yet thought it important to test the matter with regard to the specific action of light, under conditions so simple as to bring out the fundamental phenomenon unmistakably. In order to do this, I took a seedling ot Sznapis nigra in which the hypocotyl was strictly radial, and made a record of its longitudinal growth (fig. 234). Its normal rate of growth, scen in the curve as movement upwards, was at the rate of ‘O15 mm. per minute. On now applying light to the specimen on all sides at once, the growth is seen to RESPONSIVE ACTION OF PLANT-TISSUES TO LIGHT 573 undergo rapid diminution till arrested, as seen by the curve becoming horizontal at its highest point. The continued application of light now proceeds to cause a marked contrac- tion, the maximum rate of which is ‘02 mm. per minute. It is therefore to be noticed that, in this particular experiment, light not only retarded growth, but also produced an actual shortening of the plant. On the cessation of light, growth was slowly recommenced, but the average rate of growth during the sixteen minutes following the stoppage of light was only ‘OOI mm. instead of the normal rate of ‘o15 mm. per minute. This rate was, however, gradually increased in the absence of light, and after a certain interval became normal again. It must be borne in mind, in connection with this, that though the immediate and after-effects of incident light are here seen as retardation of growth, nevertheless the rate, after the lapse of a certain interval from the cessation of stimulus, may be again increased above the normal in con- sequence of the enhancement of the tonic condition of the plant, by its absorption of energy of light. In the last experiment we saw that responsive contraction not only arrested growth, but made the tissue actually shorter. In other instances the contraction and resulting retardation are not so great. Thus in a second experiment with Szvapis, the normal rate of growth was ‘02 mm. per minute, which during the continuance of light fell to one- tenth of this, or ‘002 mm. per minute. Thus the effect of light on the growing organ is always a contraction, which may in some cases induce a mere retardation, but in others culminates not even in cessation of growth, but in an actual shortening of the responding tissue. The effects thus described occur in plants which are in normal tonic condition. But we have seen that when the specimen is, on the other hand, in a sub-tonic condition, absorption of enegy in any form from outside will, at first, by increasing the internal energy, serve to accelerate growth ; and that afterwards, when the normal tonic condition has been attained, external stimulus will have the normal effect 574 PLANT RESPONSE of retarding growth. In order to verify this inference I tooka crowing flower-bud of Cvzmum Lily, which had been previously kept in the dark. And for the further purpose of detecting even the transitory variations, I used the delicate method of balanced record. On now subjecting the specimen to the stimulation of sunlight, acting on it from all sides, I observed a preliminary acceleration of growth, which lasted one minute (fig. 235). By this time the plant had evidently attained its nor- mal tonic condition, and the continued action of light resulted in a retarda- tion of growth, as seen in the rapid descent of the curve. The light was next shut off, and the after-effect of absorbed energy is seen in the con- sequent acceleration of the rate of growth above the normal. This celeration lasted for four 5’ 20725’ 30’ 35° 40° 45° . - ‘ ¢ Fic. 235. Balanced Record of Variation of Growth in Flower-bud of Cvrznwm Lily under Diffuse Stimulation of Light Continuous lines represent the effect during application of light, the dotted line on withdrawal of light. The plant was originally in a sub-tonic condition, and ac- tonic condition. application of light at x, after short latent period, induces preliminary ac- celeration of growth. After this follows the normal retardation. On withdrawal of light, in the dotted portion of the curve is seen the negative after-effect, followed by return to the normal rate of growth. A second and long-continued application of light induces retardation, followed by oscillatory response. minutes, after which the plant returned to almost its normal growth, as seen by the record approximat- ing to the horizontal. The plant may at this point be regarded as in ordinary Light was again applied, and retardation of srowth is immediately shown by the descent of the curve. There is now no preliminary acceleration of growth, as in the case when the plant was sub-tonic. Under the long-continued action of light, there is now seen the very interesting phenomenon of the induction of autonomous pulsations of the rate of growth, whose period is about ten minutes. RESPONSIVE ACTION OF PLANT-TISSUES TO LIGHT 575 Phenomenon of oscillation under long-continued stimulation.—We have seen, in Chapter XXIV.,_ that when a tissue is subjected to continuous stimulation, so that it becomes possessed of excess of energy, its response becomes pulsatory in consequence of periodic variations of its excitability. The exhibition of this variation of excita- bility we have, for the sake of convenience, designated as periodic fatigue. The occurrence of autonomous response is intimately connected with this periodic variation. In some tissues it may be exhibited only once, while in others it may be repeated indefinitely. For example, in Mzmosa, under continuous stimulation, we see a single complete pulsation, consisting of the contractile response, and the subsequent fatigue-relaxation, which looks like recovery (fig. 59). But, in the longitudinal response of the style of Uvriclis Lily to continuous stimulus, we observed two pulsations (fig. 58). And in Btophytum and Desmodium we see these repeated indefinitely, constituting what we Fic. 236. Oscillatory Re- sponse of Arsenic acted on Continuously — by response. Hertzian Radiation know as multiple or autonomous It is very interesting to note, in Taken by method of con- : : : - ductivity variation. connection with this, that even in- organic substances, under continuous stimulation, exhibit this pulsatory response. I give here a record (fig. 236) of the oscillatory response of arsenic when acted on con- tinuously by Hertzian radiation. Or we may view this induction of multiple response in a radial vegetable organ, again, from a different standpoint. By the action of stimulus, it is easy to see that antagonistic hydrostatic actions may be set up as between the excited organ and the rest of the plant. Thus, the direct effect of continuous stimulation is contraction, with increasing expul- sion of water from the excited organ into the rest of the plant. The hydrostatic reaction which this will induce in the 576 PLANT RESPONSE rest of the plant will also constantly increase. The occur- rence of oscillatory action, under these balanced and opposed forces, is what might be expected. Moreover, in the re- sponding organ itself, we see the action of opposed forces to be induced ; for while the direct effect of local stimulus is to cause contraction, the absorbed stimulus is meanwhile increasing the internal energy, the result of which is the opposite expression in expansion. Thus, from what has been said, it would appear that in all these—namely, the hydrostatic action and reaction between the excited organ and the rest of the plant, the opposed effects of external stimulus and internal energy, and the presence of an éxcess of latent energy, with periodic variations of excitability—we have so many factors, which would all contribute to a common result, in the oscillatory character of the responsive expression. But when we come to the case of anisotropic or dorsi- ventral organs, we find an additional element making for alternation of effects, for when such an organ is diffusely stimulated, we obtain a differential response. But as the constitutions of the two anisotropic halves are different, the fatigue produced on opposite sides will not be simultaneous but alternate. Similar effects will also appear under uni- lateral stimulation of a radial organ. For here, too, the organ, owing to the relatively greater fatigue of one side, becomes molecularly anisotropic, and the stimulus by its long- continued action becomes internally diffused. Similarity of responsive reaction under light and other forms of stimulation.—It has thus been fully demonstrated that the fundamental response of the plant to the stimulus of light takes place, like that to all other forms of stimulation, by contraction, leading, in the case of growing organs, to retardation of growth. Hence the unilateral stimulus of light may be expected to induce curvatures similar to those observed under other forms of stimulation, that is to say : 1, The direct effect of moderate unilateral stimulus of RESPONSIVE ACTION OF PLANT-TISSUES TO LIGHT 577 light on the growing region will be a responsive con- cavity. 2. The effect of such stimulus of light, acting on the tip of either shoot or root, will be a convexity of the same side of the responding region. 3. Strong or long-continued stimulation of light acting - on the growing region will induce a neutral or reversed effect. 4. The response, under certain conditions of continuous stimulation, may be characterised by pulsations. It has been shown in previous chapters that the responsive action of anisotropic or dorsi-ventral organs is not funda- mentally different from that of radial organs, the seeming differences being accounted for by differential action. In explaining the various effects of stimulus of light, then, the assumption of various sensibilities in the plant is unjustified. I shall attempt, therefore, to trace out the manner in which one fundamental effect of responsive contraction is made to find diverse expressions, owing to the anatomico- physiological differentiation of the responding organ. And as the supposed different specific sensibilities do not exist, I shall designate all movements and curvatures induced by light as heliotropic, the signs positive, negative, and dia- being used only for descriptive purposes. An investigation into the action of stimulus of light, then, must apply itself to the following points : 1. The effect of unilateral stimulus, of varying intensity and duration, on the tips and growing regions respectively of radial organs. 2. The induction of autonomous movement by the absorp- tion of energy of light. 3. The action of stimulus of light on molecularly aniso- tropic and on dorsi-ventral organs. 4. The direct and after-effects of light, in inducing move- ments of daily periodicity. These are the questions which will be specially dealt with in the course of the following chapters. Pa 578 PLANT RESPONSE SUMMARY The action of the diffused stimulus of light on a radial organ is, like that of other forms of stimulation, to induce a longitudinal contraction. “The action on mature does not differ from that on growing organs. Ina growing organ the induced contraction has the effect of retarding growth. The response to light may thus consist not merely of a retardation of growth, but sometimes also of an actual shortening of the responding organ. When an organ is in a sub-tonic condition, absorption of energy of light may give rise to a transient acceleration of the rate of growth; but when the plant has attained the normal tonic condition, response is by the usual retardation of growth. The after-effect of stimulus of light may consist of a simple continuation of the characteristic responsive contrac- tion or retardation of growth. This constitutes the positive after-effect. There may also be a negative after-effect, con- sisting of expansion or acceleration of the rate of growth. Under the long-continued action of light, fatigue is induced. There is sometimes an exhibition of periodic fatigue with oscillatory response. The response to light is not different from that evoked by any other form of stimulation. The various responsive movements which occur under the action of light are thus explicable without the assumption of the possession by different organs of different specific sensibilities to light. CHET ER xX Ebi POSITIVE HELIOTROPISM Introduction—Theory of de Candolle—Inadequacy of de Candolle’s theory— Definition of terms positive and negative —Darwin’s theory of modified circum- nutation—Response of terminal leaflet of Deszodiun:—Extreme sensitiveness of some plant-organs to light—Merging of multiple in continuous response— Orientation induced by light—The perceptive region in the terminal leaflet of Desmodium —Heliotropic response in radial organ Magnetically controlled recorder—Heliotropic response of hypocotyl of Szzzafzs— Recovery and theory of recti-petality. HAVING demonstrated the fact that the stimulus of light induces contraction in mature organs, and retardation of growth in growing organs, in precisely the same manner as any other form of stimulus, we shall now proceed to study in detail the various effects produced by the unilateral application of light. Such application, long continued, is seen to induce movement of the organ, in some cases towards the light, in others away from it, and in still other organs to induce a position at right angles to it. While it is doubtless convenient to distinguish such external effects as positive, negative, and dia-heliotropic, it is nevertheless unfortunate that these terms carry with them an assumption that the movements in question are due to as many distinct sensi- bilities on the part of the plant-organs. I shall, however, endeavour to show that there is in the plant but one funda- mental sensibility to light, as to other forms of stimula- tion, which finds expression in contraction under its direct action ; and that the resulting movements of the organs are dependent on the question as to whether stimulus acts directly or indirectly, unilaterally or diffusely, and also. on anatomical or physiological peculiarities of structure The complexity of the subject being very great, it has often PBZ 580 PLANT RESPONSE been found difficult to resolve a given movement into its components, and this has led to the abandonment of the attempt to relate these various movements to any single basic reaction. So far was this carried that, in spite of the well-known observation that a radial organ, illuminated with different intensities on two different sides simultaneously, bends in the direction of the more intense illumination, Sachs found himself compelled to believe that it was the direction and not the intensity of light that determined the responsive movement. Theory of de Candolle.—It is appropriate to make here a brief mention of the theory of de Candolle, which has hitherto met with ynmerited neglect. De Candolle started from the known fact that light retards growth, and explained srowth-curvature as due to the relatively greater growth of the shaded, inducing concavity of the lighted, side. This explanation of the mechanics of such movements constitutes an important advance, though it does not take full account of all the factors of the problem. This theory of de Candolle has, however, been discarded, in consequence of the difficulty which it presented of explaining the action of the negatively helictropic organs, in which the lighted side is found to be convex. Extending this theory to cases of negative helio- tropism, it was regarded as a logical inference that light should here accelerate growth. It is doubtful, however, whether such an inference is justifiable. In any case this was negatived by the researches of Miiller-Thurgau, F. Darwin, and Wiesner, who showed that light retarded general srowth in negative as well as in positive heliotropic organs. It will be shown, however, in the course of succeeding chapters, that though the circumstances which modify the response of the plant, so that it is exhibited as negative heliotropism, are somewhat complicated, yet they in no way detract from the theory of de Candolle as applied to positive heliotropism. Inadequacy of de Candolle’s theory.— The flaw in his theory lies rather in the fact that he regarded the normal POSITIVE HELIOTROPISM 581 rate of growth under shade as the active factor in growth- curvature, elongation on the lighted side being retarded, where- as in the case of positive light-curvatures the motive-power really lies in the active responsive contraction of the lighted side, the expelled water from which, reaching the opposite, may further cause an increase of growth above the normal. This fact, that it is the active contraction of the lighted, and not the passive growth of the unlighted, side that is actually the efficient cause of heliotropic curvature, will be made clear by taking an extreme case in which there is no growth. Here, if heliotropic curvature had been due simply to differ- ential growth, the occurrence of curvature would have been an impossibility. But heliotropic curvatures are observed in organs which have come to growth-standstill. Again, grass haulms, in which growth is arrested, exhibit curvatures due to the contraction of the lighted, and the renewal of growth (due to the increased turgescence caused by expelled water) on the unlighted, side, in precisely the same manner as they were observed to do under the action of gravitational stimulus. I shall now proceed to describe some typical experiments on heliotropic effects as induced by the unilateral application of light. And since it has been shown that the responses of growing organs are not essentially different from those of pulvinated organs, it will be helpful to begin by observing the effect of the unilateral application of light on the latter, especially as these have the advantage of showing relatively rapid reactions. In order to obtain a pulvinus which approximates in character to that of a radial organ, a speci- men must be selected in which the difference of excitability, as between the upper and lower halves, is as small as possible. This may be found in the pulvinus of the large terminal leaflet of Desmodium. Definition of terms ‘ positive’ and ‘ negative.’—It will be demonstrated, in this and succeeding chapters, that how- ever various in type may be the responsive movements induced by light, they are not due to the different specific 582 PLANT RESPONSE sensibilities of different organs, but can all be shown to form only special instances of a single fundamental effect. And this fundamental effect is the same in growing, in stationary, in radial, and in anisotropic organs. In describing the respon- sive actions induced by light, it will, however, be necessary to distinguish the direction of movement, in relation to the stimulating light, by clearly defined terms. I shall therefore designate all movements, of whatever organs, fowards light as positively heliotropic, and all movements away from light of whatever organs, as negatzvely heliotropic. Darwin’s theory of modified circumnutation.— Before describing the response, it is necessary to say a few words regarding Darwin’s view of heliotropic movements as a modified form of circumnutation. According to this, the already existing movement (of circumnutation) had only to be increased in some one direction, and lessened or stopped in others, in order to become heliotropic or ap-heliotropic, as the case might be ;' but, in order to prove conclusively that heliotropic curvatures were caused by the modification of the pre-existing movements, it would be necessary to show that they did not take place in those organs from which circum- nutation was absent, and this would constitute the crucial test of the theory. The difficulty of obtaining such a speci- men is, however, so great, that Darwin, although he noted the point, was unable to apply the test. I have shown elsewhere that circumnutation is only a particular manifestation of that multiple or autonomous response of plants which is due to an excess of energy, previously absorbed. In order to obtain a plant-organ com- pletely at standstill, therefore, it would be necessary to find a specimen in which there was no ‘such excess of latent energy. The fact that circumnutation was absent could then be ascertained by means of the high magnification obtainable from the recording apparatus which I have already employed. Another difficulty lay in the fact that the use of light, how- ever feeble, for purposes of observation, would be apt of itself ' Darwin, 7he Movements of Plants, p. 449. —— POSITIVE HELIOTROPISM 583 to give sufficient stimulus to initiate multiple responses which had not been present before. This might have appeared incredible had I not, in the course of my experi- ments, had reason to know how extraordinarily sensitive plants may become to the influence of light, instances of which will be found in the investigations presently to be described. In view, indeed, of the vitiation of results which might be caused in this way, I was compelled to devise special means for obviating the use of any light whatsoever for the observation of responsive curvature. Response of the terminal leaflet of Desmodium.—In order, then, to determine the important question of whether or not light will produce heliotropic movement in a plant devoid of circumnutation, I acted on the idea that the withdrawal of superfluous energy was the essential preliminary condition, and chose for my purpose, as already said, the large terminal leaflet of a specimen of Desmodium gyrans in which all movement had come to a stop—the plant having been exhausted by flowering, and by the unfavourableness of the season, which was winter. And further, in order that there should be no storage of energy derived from light, J kept this plant for one day in a dark room. To eliminate the necessity of using light for purposes of observation, I attached the leaflet by cocoon fibre to the arm of the Optic Lever Recorder. The plant itself was enclosed in a dark box, and thus protected from any access of light. Through a trap- door in the box, light for stimulation could be thrown down on the leaf at the desired moment. The preliminary absence of any autonomous movement in the plant was seen by the quiescence of the spot of light reflected from the mirror attached to the recording lever. I now subjected the terminal leaflet of the selected speci- men to light from a candle, this being thrown down on the leaflet vertically by means of a mirror, the effective distance of the candle from the leaf being twenty centimetres. The leaflet, which had previously been quiescent, began to respond after a latent period of ten seconds, and during the course of 584 PLANT RESPONSE an exposure of twenty minutes executed five complete oscillations (fig. 237). The notable points in this record are: (1) that a perfectly quiescent organ is made to give multiple response by the stimulus of light; (2) that molecular sluggishness appears to be gradually removed by the con- tinuous absorption of energy, and the successive responses exhibit an enhanced or ‘staircase’ effect ; and (3) that from the tendency of the series of curves to tilt towards the light, the organ is seen to exhibit a resultant positive movement. It will thus be seen that we have here zo¢ a modifica- tion of an existing movement, but a series of multiple responses, with a trend in a particular direction, that is to say towards the light, constituting a posi- tive heliotropic movement. In a growing organ also, which was previously de- void of circumnutation, we shall be able to observe FIG. 237. Multiple Response to Light the induction of a simi- of Terminal Leaflet of Desmodium lar movement. As in the abr moment of APE eaten, 2 erat case of the movements of JY X. 1e arrow shows the direction of light, i.e. from above. The growth, so also in those of numbers in the abscissa represent : : fie Goan Men heliotropism, we are often able to detect multiple constituent pulsations, especially at the commencement of response when stimulus is moderate. It will be understood here that, under the unilateral contraction induced in the excited side of the organ by the stimulus of light, a hydrostatic disturbance is set up, the expelled water being forced to the opposite side—a state of things calculated to show pulsation inamarked degree. The final resultant curvature, then, as we have seen under the action of other forms of unilateral stimulus also (p. 521), represents the joint effects of the concavity of the proximal and the convexity of the distal sides. When light acts on the organ continuously for some time, the POSITIVE HELIOTROPISM 585 multiple responses become more frequent by the increased absorption of energy, and their individuality is lost. The extreme sensitiveness of some plant-organs to light.—I shall now say a few words about the extraordinary sensitiveness of some plants to the stimulus of light. Darwin gives a striking example of this in a case where the cotyledons of Phalaris canariensis, after three hours of continuous exposure to a small lamp at a distance of twelve feet, became doubtfully curved towards the light, and after seven hours and forty minutes from the first exposure were plainly, though slightly, curved towards the lamp. The candle-power of the lamp is not given, but it may be taken to be about four candles. Reducing this to the standard distance of one metre, we find four candles at a distance of twelve feet (four metres approximately) to be equal to a quarter candle at a distance of one metre. If three hours’ exposure induced a doubtful curvature, then the smallest amount of light to be effective must have been 2 or °75 candle-hour; the candle-hour giving an indication of the quantity of light that had to be absorbed by the plant in order to induce a movement that was just perceptible. Now, with the terminal leaflet of Desmodium, exposure to the light of a candle at a distance of 20 cm. for ten seconds was sufficient to initiate responsive movement. This when reduced to standard conditions is equivalent to ‘07 candle- hour. In other words, we find, as far as these two experi- ments can determine the point, that the terminal leaflet of Desmodium in this experiment was at least ten times more. sensitive than Darwin’s cotyledons of Phalarzs canariensis. Darwin, however, mentions another instance which is more like the sensitiveness of which I have just given an example. He has been using a small wax taper, in order to observe the cotyledons of Phalarzs; he used this light for one or two minutes at each observation, and observed the seedlings seventeen times in the course of the day, in consequence of which he found that zigzag responsive movements had been induced. I must again point out here that the specimen of 586 PLANT RESPONSE Desmodium which exhibited such remarkable sensitiveness had been specially chosen, as being in the least favourable tonic condition. And yet I could not even strike a match near this plant without inducing responsive movements. This will indicate the extreme sensitiveness of certain plants to light, and show that it is necessary to make our observations of induced movements without its aid. The manner in which this was done will be described presently. Merging of multiple in continuous response.—We found that light acting from above on the terminal leaflet of Desmodium gyrans gave rise to multiple rea responses. As the light was acting con- stantly on the upper half, there was a cumulative contractile effect on that half. The consequence of this was a trend of the series of curves towards the light, or an incipient positive heliotropic move- ment. I shall now proceed to show how these constituent multiple movements may often, if not always, merge into one continuous movement. For this experiment I used the same Fic. 238. Response Jeaflet as in the last, the only difference of Terminal Leaflet of Desmodium to being that I now applied the strong taees Light from stimulus of sunlight from above. The Abscisea ‘gives time in’) Se5POMSE induced is seen in fig. 238. minutes, and or- During the first impact of the stimulus a dinate movement in : cailliedetece. pulsatory movement may sometimes be observed. But the response soon becomes a continuous movement upwards. Generally speaking, the constituent multiple marks are to be seen under feeble or moderate stimulation, and a continuous movement when the stimulus is strong. In the present case, the average rate of movement of the tip of the leaf was aimost I'5 mm. per minute (fig. 238). On the stoppage of light there was persistence for some time of the after-effect of light. This was succeeded by recovery. The persistence POSITIVE HELIOTROPISM 5.87 of the after-effect varies widely, depending on the condition of the tissue as well as the intensity of stimulus. Orientation induced by light.—When the leaflet, with its sensitive motile organ, is exposed to strong sunlight, the heliotropic movement continues till the organ becomes par- allel to the direction of light. The question now arises, in this as in other cases of heliotropic movement, why should the movement come toa stop when the organ reaches this parallel position ? A partial answer to this question may be found in the fact that such movements depend upon the effective intensity of light which is absorbed, and _ this effective intensity is greatest at perpen- dicular incidence, and becomes reduced to nearly zero as the rays of light are rendered more and more oblique by the responsive movement of the organ. This consideration alone, however, would not wholly explain the orientation of the organ, parallel to the direction of light, for we know that the directive impulse caused by light persists for some time, and this would cause the organ to over- shoot the parallel position. In order, Fic. 239. Response therefore, to obtain a satisfactory explana- eS bie ta tion, I undertook the following experi- Sunlight acting from ment, which, as.will be seen, completely Bde meets the difficulties of the case. The dotted portion of the curve represents I now took the same leaflet of Des- the after-effect and : ; ; recovery on _ the modiunt as was used in the previous ex- cessation of light. periments, and caused sunlight to strike the pulvinus vertically, from below upwards, by means of a suitably inclined mirror. I obtained, as will be seen (fig. 239), a continuous responsive movement downwards—i.e. towards the direction of the light. The average rate of movement of the tip of the leaf was in this case about 2°5 mm. per minute. This is somewhat greater than the upward rate of movement, 588 PLANT RESPONSE and is probably due to the fact that the excitability of the lower half of the pulvinus is slightly greater than that of the upper. We are now in a position to understand the reason of orientation; for if we suppose light to be incident from a position slightly above the pulvinus, it will curve upwards, owing to positive heliotropism, till it has become parallel with the rays of light. Should there be any over-shooting of this position owing to after-effect, the pulvinus will then begin to curve downwards, because the light will now be acting from below. Permanent equilibrium can thus only be attained when the plant-organ has become parallel to the direction of light. The perceptive region in the terminal leaflet of Des- modium.—In connection with the response of the Desmodium leaflet to light, it is interesting to note that the pulvinus is not only the responding, but also the perceptive region ; for, throwing the light on the leaflet alone, and protecting the pulvinus with an opaque shield of black paper, we find that no responsive movement takes place; conversely, if the pulvinus alone be exposed, and the rest of the leaflet shaded, we observe the normal action. Heliotropic response in radial organs.— Having observed the peculiarities of heliotropic movement in a pulvinated organ, I shall now describe the experimental arrangements for studying the same problem in non-pulvinated growing organs. It is understood that there is no essential difference between the two movements. They are both caused by the same contractile effect, due to the stimulus of light, the only difference being that, whereas in the pulvinated organ the recovery is complete, in the growing organ it remains more or less incomplete, the curvature being fixed by growth. Magnetically controlled recorder.—The great difficulty which stands in the way of accurate investigation is the question of how to take a continuous time-record of the heliotropic curvature of the growing organ without being under the necessity of using light for purposes of observation, A ie POSITIVE HELIOTROPISM 589 a procedure which, as we have seen, causes disturbance. The free end of a normally growing organ, when acted on, say by horizontal light, bends towards it. Thus the problem is to obtain a continuous time-record of this movement, from which the latent period, the actual rate of movement, the after-effect, and other related effects might be ascertained. I have been able to solve this difficulty by devising a mag- Fic. 240. Diagrammatic Representation of the Magnetically Controlled Recorder TL’, the Optic Lever, to one arm of which the plant is attached by a thread ; M, the mirror, with small magnet, Ns, attached behind. The lever is rotated to dotted position by heliotropic curvature of the plant, diagrammatically represented disproportionately magnified. netically controlled recorder, the principle of which will be understood from the accompanying diagram (fig. 240). The principal part of this recording instrument consists of a magneto-metric arrangement. Attached to a long aluminium lever, LL’, is a vertical T-piece, v. This T-piece, V, carries a reflecting mirror, behind which is a short magnet, Ns. The whole arrangement is freely suspended by a silk thread. By means of a controlling magnet not shown in the 590 PLANT RESPONSE figure, the suspended lever may be adjusted in any con- venient azimuth. The free end of the growing plant is attached to one end of the lever, L, being on its own level, at a distance of, say, 20 cm. from the line of suspension, the attached thread being at right angles to the lever. There is a slight tension of the thread, due to the magnetic force of the controlling magnet, which tends to draw the lever away from the plant. This tension, however, is very slight. When the growing organ is acted on by light in a horizontal direction, parallel to the attached thread, and therefore at right angles to the lever, then if the action of light be to induce a positive heliotropic effect, the rotation of the suspended magnetic system, owing to the pull exerted by the curving organ, will be, when seen from the top, in the direction opposite to that of the hands of a watch. If, however, the heliotropic effect be negative, the top of the growing organ will move in the opposite direction. But it has been said that there was a slight tension of the thread, owing to the action of the controlling magnet. This being now released, the lever will move to a proportionate extent in the same direction as the hands of a watch. A spot of light reflected from the mirror magnifies these movements, and a record of this moving spot of light on a revolving drum gives the response-curve. Instead of this magnetic control, it would also be possible to use, as the controlling force, the torsion of a fine metallic wire. By using a shorter lever arm and increasing the distance of the revolving recording drum from the mirror, a wide range of magnification up to 500 times may easily be obtained. For ordinary purposes a magnification of ten times is all that is necessary. The movement of the spot of light is proportionate to the heliotropic movement of the plant, at least when the amount of that movement is not excessive. Thus, know- ing the magnification produced by the system, and the rate of the revolving drum, we can determine from the a". POSITIVE HELIOTROPISM 591 response-curves the absolute movement, and the rate of such movement. I now give a description of the complete apparatus actually used (fig. 241), consisting of the Recorder and the Heliotropic Chamber, In a dark chamber is placed the plant, attached to the Lever as explained before, A portion of the vertical piece with attached mirror, M, projects outside the chamber, It will be seen that by this arrangement the Fic. 241. Heliotropic Chamber and Magnetically Controlled Recorder Heliotropic chamber seen in the middle of the apparatus. (Guide-bars to right and left carry sliding lamps, L and L’. Exposure is given by pressing key, K, which raises shutter, s. The corresponding shutter to the right is not: shown in the figure. M, mirror of the Optic Lever ; C, controlling magnet. plant can be completely protected from light, while its move. ments are at the same time recorded by the spot of light thrown from the mirror, M, upon the recording drum, without the possibility of its reaching the plant within the chamber. The chamber carries two projecting graduated arms or suide-bars, one to the right and the other to the left, over which slides the holder for a candle, or incandescent, or Nernst’s electric lamp, LL’; but if a still stronger light be 592 PLANT RESPONSE desired, sunlight may be reflected in the required direction by a mirror. From whatever source, the light can be made to strike the plant horizontally through the slit, which is usually covered with a sliding shutter, s. By manipulating a key, kK, the shutter is raised. Thus exposure may be made at any moment, and continued for the length of time desired. That part of the record which is made on the revolving drum before exposure begins, gives an indication of the quiescent condition of the plant. The moment and duration of exposure are found from corresponding marks made on the recording surface, at the instants of opening and closing the shutter. It will be seen that we have means of controlling the intensity of illumination within wide limits by (1) the use of . different sources of light as enumerated above, and (2) varia- tion of the distance of the source of light, at least when this is artificial. : Thus we are able to illuminate the plant from the right or left flanks, or from both simultaneously, and by lights. of equal or of different intensities, at will. Again, by covering the slit with a second plate, provided with suitable apertures, we are enabled to subject any part of the plant, whether tip or growing region, or both, as desired, to the stimulus of light, and thus to determine the characteristic response of each. All these considerations will show the facilities afforded by this apparatus for carrying out a great number of diverse experiments. I shall, however, content myself here with the description of a few necessary examples. Heliotropic response of hypocotyl of Sinapis.—As examples of radial organs, exhibiting the positive helio- tropic effect, I took seedlings of Szzapzs nigra. They were attached to the lever, as indicated above, and at least half an hour was permitted to elapse, in order to remove the last possible trace of excitation due to contact. The attainment of the quiescent condition was ascertained from the stationary position of the spot of light. POSITIVE HELIOTROPISM 593 In this experiment, on a seedling of Szzafzs, light was allowed to strike the growing organ horizontally. The specimen was very sensitive, and the source of light employed at first was a candle placed at a distance of 20 cm. acting on the plant for three minutes. The responsive movement began within five seconds, and though the light was cut off, there were produced three multiple responses, after which the plant underwent a complete recovery and resumed its former position. Sunlight was next applied, and a continuous move- ment towards the light was induced (fig. 242). Fic. 242. Heliotropic Response of Sinapis Fic. 243. Heliotropic Response of x Application of candle-light for three Sinapis to Sunlight minutes gave three multiple re- sponses ; } application of sunlight Dotted line shows after-effect and gave rise to continuous response. recovery on cessation of light. In another case sunlight was applied for twelve minutes. The response commenced almost immediately on application, and the average rate of movement was I mm. per minute. In this particular case, the positive after-effect persisted for _five minutes, even on the stoppage of light, after which there was a gradual recovery (fig. 243). Recovery and theory of recti-petality.— On the cessation of stimulus, there is a more or less perfect recovery of the radial organ from its induced curvature. I have already demonstrated the fact that the growing organ acts as a diffused pulvinoid. We have just seen that the primary action of light is to induce similar motile effects in both QQ 594 PLANT RESPONSE pulvinated and growing organs. We have also seen that in both, on the cessation of stimulus, there is a tendency towards recovery. In the case of the growing organ, when stimulus is moderate, recovery is fairly complete; but when the stimulus is very strong and long-continued, some part of the induced curvature is rendered permanent through fixation by growth. An attempt has been made in the case of Vochting’s Theory of Recti-petality to account for this recovery, by assuming the action of an unknown regulating power which would tend always to bring the organ back to a straight line ; but, beyond the assumption of an unknown specific power, this theory affords no explanation of the mechanism by which recovery is brought about, and I am able to adduce considerations which obviate the necessity for thus assuming the existence of any such specific agency as that of recti- petality. In a growing organ which is radial, the tip of the growing region being free, the vertical direction is that in which there is least obstruction to growth, and as long as all the lateral tensions are the same in all directions, there is no reason why the organ in the course of its upward growth should bend permanently on any one side more than on another. We have therefore the normal growth of radial organs in a straight line; but when stimulus acts unilaterally on the growing region, a sequence of events ensues, which has already been fully explained : (1) Active contraction is induced during the continuance of stimulus, on the proximal or excited side, with concomitant diminished turgidity, and retardation of growth. (2) The water thus expelled is forced, against tension, into the growing cells of the distal side, raising their turgescence and consequent rate of growth above par. (3) The curvature thus induced is maintained as long as the difference of hydrostatic pressure on the two sides is continued, by the persistent contraction of the proximal, under the action of stimulus. POSITIVE HELIOTROPISM $95 (4) When the stimulus ceases to act, the active contraction which forced the water against tension to the distal side comes to an end, and there is a rebound of the expelled water to -the proximal side. Thus the increased growth of the distal falls, and the decreased growth of the proximal rises, to the normal rate of growth. The unequal tensions on the two sides, which previously maintained the curvature, being now equalised, the organ shows a tendency to straighten itself. (5) I have also shown that a tissue which has been subjected to stimulus, having absorbed energy and _ held it latent, exhibits it on the cessation of external stimulus, in the form of a temporary negative after-effect, that is to say an acceleration of growth above the normal. Asa result o this fact, the stimulated side of the organ will show an active tendency to neutralise the previous curvature, and return to the straight line. It is thus seen, from facts which I had already established regarding the nature of the after-effect of stimulus, that the recovery of the organ is fully explained, without postulating the existence of any specific power, such as that of recti- petality. SUMMARY The responsive movement of the plant-organ towards light is due to the excitatory contraction of the side acted upon. The curvature of a growing organ towards light is brought about by the joint action of the induced concavity of the proximal and the convexity of the distal sides. The former is the result of the contraction, negative turgidity- variation, and retardation of growth caused by the stimulus. The latter comes about by the positive turgidity-variation due to forcing-in of expelled water, expansion and accelera- tion of growth of the distal side. Such responsive movements take place in organs previously devoid of circumnutation. The sensitiveness of certain plant-organs to heliotropic stimulus is very great. The terminal leaflet of Desmodium QQ2 596 PLANT RESPONSE responds under the briefest exposure to feeble candle- light. This necessitates the making of observations on heliotropic effects without the aid of light. The perceptive region for the stimulus of light in the case of the terminal leaflet of Desmodium is the pulvinus. There is no essential difference between the heliotropic response of a growing and a pulvinated organ. On the cessation of stimulus the recovery of a pulvinated organ is complete ; and this is more or less true also of the recovery of a growing organ from response, if the stimulus have not been excessive. For the explanation of this recovery in a growing organ, it is not necessary to assume the existence of any specific power such as recti-petality. The cessation of the difference of hydrostatic pressure on the two sides—such difference being only maintained during the action of stimulus— together with the accelerated rate of growth on the proximal side, which constitutes the negative after-effect, are quite sufficient to explain the recovery from induced curvature. CHARTER, XLII NEGATIVE HELIOTROPISM Incomplete parallelism between actions of light and of gravitation—Theoretical considerations — Recording microscope — Negative heliotropic curvature induced by stimulation of the tips of root and shoot—Intermediate phases between positive and negative heliotropic response: (a) neutralisation by transverse transmission ; (4) neutralisation by transverse transmission, with multiple response—Localised sensitiveness to light and transmission of excitatory effect—Negative heliotropism of a radial organ—Gradual transition from positive to negative, through intermediate phase of neutrality—Apparent heliotropic insensitiveness of certain tendrils—Negative heliotropism of tendril of Vitis. WE have seen in the chapter on the response due to gravita- tion, that the responsive curvature of the root is opposite in character to that of the stem, this fact having led to the assumption of specific sensibilities as characteristic of the root-tip. It was there shown, however, that the responsive characteristics of the root were not actually different from those of the shoot, and that the differences in their observed responses were simply a consequence of the fact that in the one case the stimulus of gravity acted indirectly, and in the other directly, upon the responding growing organ. This assumption that the root possessed a definite sensitiveness characteristically different from that of the stem, was ap- parently supported by certain differences in heliotropic action also, as between shoot and root; for example, while the hypocotyl of Szzapzs bends towards the light, the root is found to bend away from. it. Incomplete analogy between action of light and gravitation.—But I have already explained the fact that the supposed analogy is false ; for while the stimulus of gravity acts, in the case of the root, only on a restricted area of 598 PLANT RESPONSE the tip, the stimulus of light is not necessarily restricted in the area of its action. Again, whereas the stimulation caused by statolithic particles is moderate, that caused by light may be of any degree of intensity. The fact that there is no true extended analogy between the action of light and that of gravitation, is seen from the fact that, while gravitation in the case of the root induces a movement opposite to that induced in the stem, in the case of light this is not always so; for though,a few roots turn away from light, in others there is either no resultant movement, or movement towards the light. Again, while the shoot makes a definite curvature with reference to the direction of gravity, in the case of light we shall observe that though under moderate stimulation it turns towards it, yet it will sometimes under stronger stimulation be found to move away. Theidea that positive and negative heliotropic curvatures are due to two distinct sensibilities could not be better disproved than by the fact, which will be demonstrated shortly, that the same organ can be made under different conditions of illumination to exhibit the two opposite effects. Discarding, then, the theory of any specific sensibility, we shall now proceed to show how the movement away from the stimulating light, the so-called negative heliotropic curvature, is brought about. Theoretical considerations.—From the movements al- ready demonstrated (p. 535) as taking place in plant-organs in response to stimulus unilaterally applied, we can see the possibility of such movement becoming negative, or away from stimulus, under three different conditions : (1) Under longitudinal transmission of the indirect effect of stimulus, when, for example, moderate stimulus is applied to the tip of either shoot or root. (2) Under transverse conduction of the direct excitatory effect of stimulus to the distal side of a radial organ, the proximal side being fatigued by excessive stimulation ; and (3) Under the transverse transmission of excitation to the distal side of an anisotropic organ, the distal side being the NEGATIVE HELIOTROPISM 599 more excitable. In this last case, we may obtain a very pronounced negative response in consequence of the relatively greater natural excitability of the distal side. We shall see in the course of this and the following chapters how heliotropic movements other than positive are actually brought about under these different conditions, and in the present chapter we shall study cases which are illustrative of the first two, Fic. 244. Microscope Recorder Plant mounted in cubical glass trough with root in water. Light strikes root, R, unilaterally from the right side. Movement of root observed by microscope, M, the inclined transparent disc of glass, G, giving at the same time the reflected image of the recording pen, P. Recording microscope.—Since the growing root has to be kept in water, for the purpose of studying the phases of its responsive curvature, the method hitherto employed of obtain- ing records by the Optic Lever is inapplicable. I therefore devised a different method of observation—- that of the Record- ing Microscope (fig. 244). The method of record will be understood from the figure, where in a cubical glass trough a piece of the stem of Bindweed, with its water root, R, is securely fixed on the surface of the water. Light is made to 600 PLANT RESPONSE strike the root unilaterally, say from the right side. This pencil of light may be so thrown as to act locally on the root- tip, or on the growing region, or on both at the same time. The movement of the root towards or away from light is observed through the microscope focussed on the tip. The eye-piece end of the microscope has a disc of glass adjusted at an angle of 45° to the vertical. The observer sees the tip of the root directly through the transparent disc, and at the same time the reflected image of the recording point of the pen, lying against the revolving drum below. The two images are at the beginning of the experiment coincident, and the responsive movement of the tip of the root, which takes place afterwards, is easily followed by the observer with the recording pen. Thus we obtain the response-record on the moving surface. This method of the recording microscope can always be used when attachment to the Optic Lever is not possible or not desired. Negative heliotropic curvature induced by stimulation of the tips of root and shoot.—I have by this method ob- tained various records of the responses of the root and shoot to the unilateral stimulus of light applied at the tip. Of these I shall give, as a typical example, the record of the root of a seedling of Szzapts nigra, suitably mounted in the cubical trough by means of a cork. The curve seen to the left of fig. 245 represents the negative movement, or movement away from light, of this root, when the tip alone was unilaterally stimulated. This movement was due therefore to the indirect action of the stimulus on the growing responding region. After a period of rest in darkness I next took a record of its movement resulting from the direct unilateral illumination of the growing region. I now ob- tained a positive responsive curvature, as seen to the right of fig. 245. It will be noticed that this particular movement was relatively smaller than the preceding. We must here remember that the receptivity of an organ is not the same in all its different parts, and the greater negative response induced in this case by the indirect action of stimulus on the “NEGATIVE HELIOTROPISM 601 tip is probably due to the higher degree of receptivity possessed by that part of the organ. In taking a third record in a case in which both tip and growing region were simul- taneously subjected to unilateral stimulation of light, I found that a resultant responsive movement was induced, which was away from light. That this negative movement, induced by stimulation of the root-tip, is not due to any specific sensitiveness of the root as such, is seen from the fact that on local stimulation of the tip of the shoot, e.g. the flower-bud of Cvocus, I obtained a responsive movement away from, whereas uni- lateral stimulation of the - growing region of the pe- : duncle induced a move- 0° 10° 20° 30° a ment towards, light. Gr er eeeas ; Apart from this pos- sible factor, however, of the greater receptivity of the tip, there is another, which tends to make the positive curvature of the growing region of the 0° 10° 20° 30’ 40° { FIG. 245. Record of Response of Root of Sinapis nigra The curve seen to the left shows the negative root relatively ineffec- response due to stimulation of root-tip. ; . ; i The curve to the right exhibits positive tive: This region, being response on stimulation of growing region. acted on unilaterally by light, the proximal excitation often passes by conduction to the distal side, thus neutralising the first positive action. Instances of this will be given in greater detail presently. The negative curvature induced by the action of the tip, depending as it does on the indirect transmission of stimula- tion, requires as acondition the relative non-conductivity of the intervening tissue to the passage of true excitation. Hence, if the conductivity of such a tissue be not sufficiently feeble, or if the intensity of stimulus be too great, we shall find that the direct effect of stimulus is transmitted to the growing organ, and a positive curvature is induced. This 602 PLANT RESPONSE explains the positive heliotropic curvature exhibited by many roots. Intermediate phases between positive and negative heliotropic response.—I shall next proceed to demonstrate the induction of negative heliotropic movements in radial shoots, a phenomenon which, for reasons already explained, has no parallel in the case of geotropic action (p. 544). As it has already been said that there is no specific sensibility which determines the positive or negative character of the heliotropic response, it would be interesting to trace out the transitions by which the normal positive is gradually transformed into the negative movement. We have seen that when stimulus is applied unilaterally to a growing region, the positive curvature at first induced is jointly due to the contraction caused by direct stimulation of the proximal and the expan- sion caused by the indirect stimulation of the distal; but when the stimulus is strong or long-continued, excitation is transmitted from the proximal to the distal, the contraction of which latter now neutralises the first effect. Hence the normal positive curvature disappears. (a) Neutralisation by transverse transmtssion.—The con- siderations just related explain the curious anomaly that has been observed, by which, while feeble or moderate stimulus of light, or interrupted light, gives rise to well-marked positive heliotropic curvature, the continuous application of stronger light induces a relatively feeble effect. Thus under moderate lighting we often observe strong heliotropic curvature, which disappears under strong sunlight. The curvature induced is, as we have seen, due to the afferentzal action of unilateral stimulus, on the proximal and distal sides ; but when a strong light is used the stimulus becomes internally diffused, and the differential effect on the two sides is reduced in amount or vanishes altogether. Such internal diffusion is due to the fact that, owing to the weak transverse conductivity of the tissue, while a feeble stimulus is not conducted across it, a stronger stimulus is. This consideration, together with the fact that the conductivity of a tissue undergoes seasonal variation, NEGATIVE HELIOTROPISM 603 will be found to offer a satisfactory explanation of various anomalies in heliotropic response. Szzapzs, for example, exhibits a strong positive effect in winter, while in hot weather its action is very feeble. It may be supposed that this is due, in some unknown way, to a greater rapidity of growth in warm than in cold seasons. That this, however, cannot be the reason, will be seen from the fact which I have demonstrated, that that contractile response of the plant to external stimulus on which curvature depends is greatest when the rate of growth is at its optimum. The real explanation lies in the fact that the neutralisation, or reversal of normal positive response, caused by transverse conduction, takes place more easily in warmer seasons, the general con- ducting power being then great. This accounts for the feebler positive response in summer, which culminates in certain instances in an actual reversal into negative (p. 623). (6) Neutralisation by transverse transmission, with multiple response.—Thus if stimulus be sufficiently strong or long- continued, the positive curvature will become neutralised, and the organ will return to its original position. I have, however, observed an interesting modification of this neu- tralisation, in which it is attended by oscillatory move- ments to and fro about the mean position. We have seen that unilateral stimulus, when its action is long continued, becomes diffused, and thus both sides of the organ become excited. The tissue, moreover, is now possessed of an excess of energy—a condition conducive to the production of multiple response. This fact, together with the periodic and alternate variation of excitability on the two sides, is then found to give rise to oscillatory movements of the kind described. I give below a record which shows the initiation of these oscillatory movements when the organ had been too long subjected to unilateral stimulus. It will be remembered that the pulvinus of the terminal leaflet of Desmodium executes a positive heliotropic movement, the record of which has been given in fig. 238. In winter, when the conductivity of the tissue is feeble, the leaflet curves towards 604 PLANT RESPONSE the light to the maximum extent possible, and remains in that position as long as the light acts. But we have seen that in summer the stimulus is more likely to be internally transmitted to the distal side, the positive effect being thus gradually neutralised. Thus, in the course of an experiment during the summer on the pulvinus of the terminal leaflet of Desmodium, 1 found, on subjecting it to sunlight from above, that for the first forty minutes the leaflet rose continuously, its tip having moved during that time through a little more than 4 cm. After this there was induced, instead of the continuous movement upwards, a pulsatory movement up and down (fig. 246). After a series of such movements the leaflet was gradually depressed, the former positive curvature being thus neutralised. The supposed localisation of sensitiveness to light, and the transmission of excitatory effect. I have fully explained the man- MMe Meme ner in which the effect of stimulus Big BAe! Upsatee TElatotrenio of light applied at a given point is Movement of Terminal Leaf- transmitted to the distant growing let of Desmodium Converted : ” by Strong and too Long- Organ, and the mechanics by which continued Stimulus of Light the curvature is induced. In con- into Oscillatory Movement } . : : nection with this, a peculiar phe- nomenon has been observed, which has led to the belief that in seedlings, like that of Avena sativa, the zone for the perception of heliotropic stimulus is confined to the upper region, or tip of the shoot. This conclusion is based on Darwin’s observations on the unilateral effect of light on these seedlings. It was found that, generally speaking, when the lower part of the cotyledon was alone exposed to the unilateral light—the upper part being covered with a cap of tinfoil or with an opaque glass tube—there was little curvature induced ; but when such light was allowed to act on the upper part of the seedlings the curvature was con- NEGATIVE HELIOTROPISM 605 siderable. From this it was concluded that sensitiveness to light was mainly confined to the upper part of the plant, and that this determined the curvature ; but to this conclusion, that it was the upper rather than the lower part that was sensitive to light, Darwin found and recorded several excep- tions, which he regarded as inexplicable. In the case of six seedlings, for instance, of which the upper parts were covered with opaque shields, there was as much curvature induced as in seedlings which were unshielded. In these, therefore, there must have been sensitiveness in the lower parts also, thus negativing the conclusion, drawn from other and more numerous experiments, that it was characteristic of the upper alone. In order to see if these discrepancies were not capable of explanation, I undertook an investigation into the heliotropic action of light on the seedlings of Avena sativa. The method of screening the upper part of the seedlings from light, which has usually been employed by Darwin and others, labours under the disadvantage that the weight and contact of the tinfoil or the blackened glass tube are not unlikely themselves to set up a certain mechanical irritation, which may have the effect of causing an unknown disturbance in the result. I was therefore desirous of keeping the delicate seedling free from the irritating contact of caps in the course of my own experiments. For this reason I arranged for the iocalised application of light on upper or lower or both parts of the organ at will, by the method which has already been described of throwing a pencil of light on the required spot in the plant placed in the heliotropic chamber (p. 592). The resultant movement of the organ was now continuously observed and recorded, by means of the Recording Microscope which has been described. For the sake of clearness I may here forestall matters, by saying that the observed results fall under two types, according to whether the given specimen possesses feeble or high conductivity—that is to say, power of transmitting stimulus to a distance.. Taking first the case in which the 606 PLANT RESPONSE organ possesses feeble conductivity, I have found that when the extreme tip was stimulated unilaterally, by using a pencil of light from a sixteen candle-power incandescent electric lamp, the result was a negative movement—that is to say, a move- ment away from light. This is exactly parallel to the response to stimulus of the unopened flower-bud of Crocus and the root-tip of Szzapzs, in both of which the indirect effect at the growing region, of stimulus applied on the tip, was seen to be a convexity of the side acted upon, with consequent negative movement of the tip. I next applied unilateral stimulus to the same specimen a little lower down, and now, owing to the better conductivity of this part of the tissue, the excitation itself was transmitted to the growing region, inducing concavity and positive heliotropic movement. The same effect was found to be produced when stimulus was applied on the growing region itself. It must be remembered that in this case the con- ducting power of the tissue is not high, hence there is no transmission of stimulus to the distal side, by which, as we have seen, the positive curvature would be neutralised. The long-continued action of light on one side here tends only to increase the positive curvature toa maximum. Thus when one side of the entire seedling is acted upon by light, while the response of the extreme tip tends to induce a slight negative, all the other parts, from immediately below it to the growing region, conspire together to exhibit a much stronger positive heliotropic action. The result is therefore a movement towards the light. Thus we see that in the case of seedlings having feeble conductivity the curvature will be positive, whether it is the lower part only or the entire plant which is exposed to the one-sided action of light. In this fact we find the explanation of those exceptional cases observed by Darwin, in which the seedling was found to bend towards the light, in spite of the upper part being covered. We shall next take up that type of response in which the tissue of the specimen is rather better conducting. In this case, when the upper part of the organ is locally stimulated, NEGATIVE HELIOTROPISM 607 the excitation is longitudinally transmitted to the growing region lower down, and induces a concavity there which increases with the duration of the stimulus; but if the stimulus of light be applied directly on the growing region itself, instead of on the upper part of the specimen, then, by reason of the transverse transmission of excitation to the distal side, we obtain a state of things in which there is no resultant curvature at all. In this case, then, the direct effect of stimulus on the proximal side of the growing region is balanced or neutralised by the transmitted effect on the distal side ; but this condition of balance will be upset if the uni- lateral stimulus of light, hitherto acting on the growing region alone, be allowed to act simultaneously on the upper part of the specimen also. The longitudinally transmitted stimulus from the upper part being now added to the direct excitation of the proximal side of the growing region, causes an over-balance of responsive effect on that side, resulting in a positive heliotropic curvature. The fact that there is no heliotropic movement, when only the lower part of the seedling is unilaterally acted on by stimulus, is thus not due to any absence in that region of heliotropic sensibility, but to the neutralisation of the proximal effect by the equal excita- tion of the distal. Such transmission of stimulation along the length of the organ is observed to take place in a specially marked manner in the cotyledon of graminaceous plants. We may account for this by the fact that such organs are parallel-veined—that is to say, the fibro-vascular elements, which we already know as good conductors of excitation, run along their length. This is no doubt the reason of their ready transmission of stimulus to a distance. Negative heliotropism of a radial organ.—We have seen that when moderate stimulus acts unilaterally on a growing organ a positive curvature is induced, and that, with stronger or long-continued stimulation, this reaches the distal side, producing neutralisation. We shall now proceed te trace out the continuity of responsive heliotropic effects, from the positive curvature to the negative, through the inter- 608 PLANT RESPONSE mediate phase of neutralisation. We have seen that at a certain definite intensity of illumination the excitations of proximal and distal sides, balancing each other, cause neutralisation. If now the intensity of stimulating light be further increased, it is easy to see that while the stimulation transmitted to the distal side, with the concomitant contrac- tion, is being increased, the excitatory contraction of the proximal will be at the same time decreased, owing to the fatigue brought on by over-stimulation. The result then will be the greater contraction of the distal side, with a consequent negative heliotropic curvature of the organ. These considerations show the mechanical action of heliotropic stimulus in causing : (1) positive heliotropic curva- ture under moderate illumination ; (2) the neutralisation of this action under stronger illumination ; and (3) the conversion of the normal positive into negative when illumination is excessive. Thus the observation made by Oltmanns, on young seedlings of Lepzdium, subjected to varying intensities of light, an observation of which there has been hitherto no satisfactory explanation, is.fully accounted for. Oltmanns subjected a row of seedlings of Lepzdzum to the action of sunlight, diverging from the focus of a lens. The seedlings nearest the focus were thus subjected to the strongest stimu- lation, those further from this point being under gradually decreasing intensities of light. It was then found that the seedlings nearest the focus, which were subjected to the strongest degree of light, exhibited negative heliotropic curvature, while others, further away, and therefore subjected to less intense illumination, did not show any effect at all (neutralisation), and others again, which were still further away, and therefore under only moderate intensity of illumination, exhibited positive curvature. Gradual transition from positive to negative, through intermediate phase of neutrality.—I shall, however, give a still more conclusive verification of the theoretical inferences which I have just set forth regarding the gradual transition of positive heliotropic response into negative, through the NEGATIVE HELIOTROPISM 609 intermediate neutral, in consequence of the increasing internal diffusion of stimulus with increasing intensity of stimulating light. This I shall do by a continuous record taken from a single plant under changing conditions of increasing illumina- tion, in which record, further, we shall be able to follow all the phases of responsive change, from positive to negative. Taking a hypocotyl of Szzapzs nigra, | subjected it to the unilateral action of light from a sixteen-candle-power incan- descent electric lamp, placed at a distance of 10 cm. from the specimen. The plant, hitherto quiescent, began to move towards the light,as shown by the up curve in the record (fig. 247), the maxi- mum being attained in the course of fifty minutes. The intensity of incident stimulus was now in- creased, by bringing the lamp toa distance of 6 cm. from the specimen, at the Fic. 247.. Response of Hypocotyl of moment marked with the Sinapis nigra downward arrow. It will The first curve shows positive response be seen that this resulted induced by incandescent,'electric lamp ; at distance of 10cm. Increased in- in a process of neutralisa- tensity of light applied at arrow (J) = : by bringing lamp to distance of 6 cm. tion of the preceding re- causes neutralisation. Reversal, or nega- sponse, and that this tive response, when sunlight applied became complete in the is course of a further exposure of seventy minutes, the hypo- cotyl being then erect and free from curvature, having thus placed itself at right angles to the incident light. Still stronger illumination of sunlight was now applied, at the point marked x. This induced, as is seen in the down curve, a very marked reversed or negative heliotropic response. Thus, in other words, in an identical organ, under different conditions of illumination, the plant turning towards the light exhibits the poszteve heliotropic, at right angles to the light the dza-helotrop~ic, and away from the light the zegatzve RR 610 PLANT RESPONSE heliotropic effect, thus conclusively proving that the induction of these three effects is not due to the presence of three distinct and characteristic sensibilities. These different responsive movements are thus traced to a single pheno- menon of contractile response. This may be made equally clear from another point of view, as illustrations namely of the general law that response takes place by the contraction of the more excited, or the relatively more excited. With mcderate stimulation it is the proximal side of the organ that is excited, and, becoming concave, gives rise to a positive heliotropic curvature. On the application of somewhat stronger stimulation, however, when excitation is transmitted to the distal side, there isa case in which the excitation of the proximal and distal are equal, and the differential excitation being thus zero, there is no resultant response. The organ, standing thus at right angles to the light, and moving neither towards nor away from it, will now appear to be either dia-heliotropic or else irresponsive to heliotropic stimulus. But under still stronger stimulation two effects are in- duced simultaneously : (1) a physiological anisotropy, by the fatigue and loss of excitability of the proximal, in conse- quence of which the distal becomes the more excitable ; and (2) the internal diffusion of the stimulus, which, now acting on the physiologically anisotropic organ, induces concavity of the distal, that is to say a negative heliotropic curvature. 1 shall now proceed to give further examples of that transverse transmission of excitation, in consequence of which an organ appears either to be irresponsive to heliotropic stimulus or to give negative response. Organs apparently insensitive to light.— Many vegetable organs exhibit no resultant movement under the action of light, and are therefore supposed to be insensitive to it ; but this inference could be accepted only on the theory of a specific sensibility, in which case some organs would be without it, while in others it would be characterised by certain inherent positive or negative properties. We have —— a NEGATIVE HELIOTROPISM 611 seen, however, that such a theory is untenable. The apparent absence in some organs of sensibility to light may perhaps, then, be explicable, not as want of sensitiveness, but as the neutralisation of effect, by the equal excitation of the two opposite sides. In the case of certain seedlings of Avena, it has already been shown that the so-called insensitiveness of the lower part of the organ was due to this cause. Many tendrils, again, according to Mohl and others, are heliotropically insensitive. Thus, for example, on subjecting the tendril of Passzflora to lateral sunlight, there is practically no responsive movement ; but as the tendril is a highly conducting organ, we might expect that its responsive movement would be neutralised by the transverse transmission of excitation. It occurred to me that this question, as between a characteristic insensitiveness, and a sensitiveness with equal excitation of two sides, might be tested by artificial reduction of the conducting power by cooling. Under such circumstances, if any sensitiveness existed, a one-sided excitation by light would remain localised, and induce concavity, or positive heliotropic move- ment. On carrying out this experiment, I found that the selected tendril of Passiflova now exhibited a marked positive heliotropic movement by the induced concavity of the side acted upon. Negative heliotropism of tendril of Vitis.— The tendril of Vztzs is adduced as the type of those organs which exhibit the negative heliotropic effect. 1 therefore undertook an investigation on this organ, to determine whether it would not be possible to explain its negative movement without postulating the existence, in its case, of a specific heliotropic sensibility of negative sign. When sunlight strikes it on one side it is found that it moves away from the light. If, now, this movement be really due to the intensity of stimulus, causing it to be rapidly conducted to the distal side, and at the same time giving rise to the fatigue of the proximal, then _ we should expect that the application of moderate unilateral illumination would induce the positive heliotropic movement. RR2 612 PLANT RESPONSE The crucial test would thus lie in the observation of the responsive movement under moderate unilateral stimulation. I placed atendril in a dark room, and subjected it to light of moderate intensity from a sixteen-candle-power electric lamp, placed at a distance of 15 cm. This induced an active movement of the tendril towards the light, or positive helio- tropic response. I then brought the lamp nearer, to a distance of 5 cm., thus increasing the intensity of light. The active positive movement was now quickly reversed into a move- ment away, or negative heliotropic response. This experiment once more demonstrates the fact that positive and negative heliotropic responses are not due to two specific sensibilities of opposite sign. The negative heliotropic curvature in an organ originally radial, which we have just studied, was due to the physio- logical anisotropy induced by the stimulus itself; but there are organs in which anisotropy is already developed in various degrees of perfection, and in them we shall be able to observe varying intensities of this negative heliotropic effect. This will be discussed in detail in succeeding chapters. SUMMARY Negative curvature is induced under the action of light in two different ways: (1) by the indirect effect of the moderate stimulation of the tip of root or shoot; and (2) by the transversely transmitted effect of strong or long-continued stimulus acting on the distal side of an organ, when the proximal has become fatigued. The parallelism between geotropic and heliotropic effects is thus incomplete. The direct effect of stimulus, unilaterally applied, may be longitudinally transmitted, and cause responsive. movement in the distant growing region. The unilateral effect of stimulating light on the growing region varies with its intensity, as follows : 1. Moderate intensity induces the normal positive re- sponsive movement. e NEGATIVE HELIOTROPISM 613 2. (a) Stronger or long-continued stimulus, on account of its internal diffusion by transverse transmission, causes neutralisation of effect. In other words, the organ, after first moving towards the light, returns to its original position at right angles to it, or assumes the dia-heliotropic attitude. This neutralisation, depending as it does on transverse con- duction of stimulus, can only take place when the stimulus is very strong, or when the organ is highly conducting. The first of these considerations explains the fact that while moderate stimulation causes positive curvature, stronger stimulation has no resultant effect. The effect of the second factor (the higher conductivity of the tissue), which is brought about by a warmer season, is seen when the transversely transmitted effect causes neutralisation. The apparent absence of heliotropic effect in various tendrils is due not to any want of sensibility, but to this neutralisation by transverse conduction of the effect of stimulus. (6) The transverse transmission of stimulus to the distal side is sometimes attended by oscillatory responsive move- ments, owing to periodic or alternate fatigue. 3. With still stronger unilateral stimulation, the organ becomes for the time being anisotropic, owing to the fatigue of the proximal side. The internally diffused stimulus then induces negative curvature, through the relatively greater excitability and contraction of the distal half of the organ. The negative heliotropic movement of the tendril of Vz¢zs is explained by these considerations. This tendril, under moderate unilateral stimulus, exhibits positive heliotropic movement; but stronger stimulation, in consequence of transverse transmission and unilateral fatigue, gives rise to negative heliotropic movement. The statement that the different responsive curvatures brought about by light are not due to different sensibilities possessed by different organs, is proved by the fact that the same organ exhibits continuous changes, from positive to negative through neutral, under different intensities of stimulation. CHAPTERGOAELY “ EFFECT OF INVISIBLE RADIATION AND EMANATIONS Effect of temperature and its variations—Demonstration of fundamental effect of thermal radiation on growth—Response to successive uniform stimuli of thermal radiation—Effect of continuous unilateral stimulation—Effect of electrical waves on grcwth—Response of A/imosa to electric radiation— Action of high frequency Tesla current. WE have now studied the curvature effects induced in plants by those ethereal vibrations that lie within narrow limits, and are known as visible light. There are, however, other vibra- tions outside this range, the ultra-violet and the infra-red. The excessively quick vibrations beyond violet are known to produce very marked heliotropic effects. But below the red, again, we have comparatively long waves which give rise to thermal, and others, still longer, to electrical radiation. In studying the curvature-effects on plants of invisible radiations of low frequency, it is necessary to distinguish carefully between the action of radiation as such and the subsidiary effect of temperature. Effect of temperature and its variation.—In this in- vestigation it becomes especially important to distinguish the temperature from the radiation-effect, and I shall pre- sently describe a very decisive experiment by which the effects of the two may be clearly distinguished. The effect of temperature up to the optimum is, as we have seen, to increase the internal energy of the plant, in consequence of which there is an acceleration of the rate of growth; but variation of temperature acts as an external stimulus, and. would thus be effective in inducing ‘a transient retardation of srowth. A part of the stimulus, however, is held latent, as EFFECT OF INVISIBLE RADIATION AND EMANATIONS 615 we have seen, in the tissue, so long as the temperature is below the optimum, to give rise later to an acceleration of the rate of growth. Hence, frequent variations of tempera- ture below the optimum will, by reason of these alternate retardations and accelerations, produce little total effect on the rate of growth; but above the optimum, the stimu- lating action of variation of temperature will retard growth, and as there is here no latent factor, this will not be made up by any subsequent acceleration (p. 461). Hence, the total effect of such variation will be a retardation. This consideration explains the different conclusions to which observers have been led as to the effect of frequent variation of temperature on growth. Demonstration of fundamental effect of thermal radia- tion on growth.—In the usual experiments on the effect of thermal radiation on the induction of growth-variation, a difficulty arises in distinguishing between the effect of thermal radiation and that of temperature. In order, then, to determine the fundamental effect of thermal radiation, the experiment must be so arranged that the radiation whose effect is to be observed causes no change of temperature. I have been able to accomplish this by mounting the growing organ in a plant chamber surrounded by a wide heating coil of platinum wire. Between the coil and the specimen there is a cylinder of mica, which is opaque to thermal radiation. By means of a string attached to it from above, this cylinder may be alternately lifted and lowered. ~ The plant is attached to the Crescograph, and a balanced record is taken of its growth when the shield is down, and when, by maintaining a current through the heating coil, the chamber has already been brought to a steady temperature of 34°C. As every- - thing inside the chamber has now attained a steady tempera- ture, the movement of the shield up and down will produce no change in this condition. By now raising the mica shield, we can subject the specimen to the action of thermal radiation, which proceeds from the heated spiral wire, with- out producing any variation of temperature. When the 616 PLANT RESPONSE mica shield is again dropped the action of radiation on the organ is cut off. Experimenting in this manner, and obtaining a horizontal record with the shield down, we find, when the shield is lifted, that there is at once an upsetting of the balance, which indicates a retardation of growth. When the shield is once more allowed to drop, the deflected record becomes again horizontal, indicating the restoration of the original rate of growth. This experiment conclusively shows that radiation by itself acts as an external stimulus, retarding the rate of growth. It will be remembered that this is quite distinct from the effect of temperature, which, up to the optimum, always induces the opposite result, namely, an enhancement of the rate of growth. The same distinction is also to be borne in mind in dealing with the excitatory effect of light, for incident light also has the twofold effect of causing external stimulation and at the same time raising the tem- perature of the tissue. Response to successive uniform stimuli of thermal radiation.—Having thus demonstrated the fundamental action of thermal radiation, we shall now study the effect of the unilateral appli- cation of this form of stimulus, which will be found similar to that caused by visible light. The responsive effect of successive uniform stimulations may be studied by placing a - Fic. 248. Responses to Successive Uniform Stimuli of Thermal Radiation in Pistil of AZusa V-shaped platinum wire with its point opposite to the region of growth, and heating it periodically by definite currents of short duration; the flashes of thermal radiation thus produced bring about the usual responsive concavity. These responses and recoveries are recorded in the usual manner. I give in fig. 248 a series of such responses EFFECT OF INVISIBLE RADIATION AND EMANATIONS 617 of the pistil of Musa, in some specimens of which the growing region is found to be narrow and sharply defined. In this case the V-shaped radiator was placed opposite to this region. In these responses we see that there is a con- siderable amount of recovery after each transient stimulation, and also a certain degree of fatigue. Effect of continuous unilateral stimulation.—We shall next study the effect of continuous unilateral stimulation. For this experiment I took the hypocotyl of Yamarindus wndica. As the growing region in this case is extended, Fic. 249. Response of Hypocotyl of Zamarindus indica to Continuous Stimulation of Thermal Radiation I used a thermal radiator, which consisted of a linear plati- num wire, 5 cm. in length, placed parallel to one side of the growing region. Under the action of continuous uni- lateral radiation, an increasing positive curvature—that is to say, towards the stimulus—was induced, till a maximum effect had been attained (fig. 249). With specimens of other plants I obtained either the neutralisation or the reversal of this curvature, as it might happen, in consequence of the internal diffusion of stimulus to the distal side. This neutra- lisation is brought about by equal excitation of the proximal and distal sides, in precisely the same manner as in the 618 PLANT RESPONSE case of light. It is seen in the return of the organ to its original position, or by oscillations about that position as the mean ; but in ‘cases where the proximal becomes fatigued, the responsive contraction of the distal gives rise to a nega- tive curvature. In all these cases we see thermal radiation inducing the same curvature effects—positive, neutral, and negative—as were found to be induced by visible light. Effect of electric waves on growth.—I shall next describe the effect of Hertzian waves in inducing responsive curvatures ; and first, in order to obtain the fundamental effect on growth itself, I took an electric radiator consisting of a rod 5 cm. mim 047 e05+ Fic. 250. Effect of Electric Waves on Growth Connection with electric vibrator at downward arrow (J) is seen to arrest growth and to induce contraction, by which the specimen undergoes an actual shortening. in length, excited by oscillatory discharge from a Ruhmkorff’s coil. The specimen was a flower-bud of Crzzum Lily, whose upper and lower ends were connected by means of thin wires with the two ends of the electric radiator. The natural growth-record under unbalanced conditions was first taken (fig. 250), and the specimen was now subjected to the action of electric waves at the point marked in the record with a downward arrow. It will be seen that this gradually diminished the rate of growth, till at the end of five minutes it was completely arrested ; and as the action of the electric waves continued, the contractile effect by which growth is arrested is seen to be carried further, actually bringing about a shortening in the length of the specimen. EFFECT OF INVISIBLE RADIATION AND EMANATIONS 619 Response of Mimosa to electric radiation.— In the last case, in order to subject the specimen to the intense action of electric waves, the radiator was electrically connected with the responding organ, which was diffusely stimulated by it, and exhibited a longitudinal response of contraction. If, however, we wish to observe the effect of the unilateral action, it will be necessary that the radiation shall act on one side only. A difficulty is here met with, however, owing to the relatively greater length of these electric waves, which, like those of sound, do not cast shadows, but curl round corners. In order to produce unilateral action, then, the length of these waves has to be reduced to a minimum. By using small spheres of platinum as the source of radiation, I succeeded in obtaining short electric waves of about I cm. in length ; but the intensity of such radiation is somewhat feeble, and with them I could only occasionally obtain responsive curvatures of growth. I was, however, more successful in obtaining responsive effects from the pulvinus of Mimosa. When the small radiator which I have described was placed at a distance of a few centimetres from the lower half of a pulvinus, and when the radiator was allowed to act, a depression of the leaf of this plant occurred after an exposure of nearly half a minute to the continuous action. It should be mentioned that for the demonstration of this effect it is the younger leaves which are suitable, the older not being sufficiently sensitive. With regard to the electric waves, it is to be borne in mind that to them water is opaque, while ebonite is highly transparent. Hence the interposition of a sheet of ebonite, between the radiator and the plant, does not stop the responsive action, while a parallel-sided trough of water in the same place would effectually prevent the passage of the rays, and thereby put an end to the action. Action of high frequency Teslacurrent. —If one electrode of a Tesla coil be put in connection with the growing organ, this electric variation of high frequency is found to cause an arrest of growth. Even the contiguity of wires carrying 620 PLANT RESPONSE these high frequency currents is often found to retard growth. It is probable that this is due to certain material emanations that proceed from the wire. A coil of iron wire was made to surround the growing organ, and during the excitation of this coil the normal growth-rate was found to be retarded ; but there was no such retardation when a glass cylinder was interposed between the specimen and the coil. During the short period of the experiment there was but little rise of temperature within the coil, and the retardation of growth could not have been due to any thermal radiation. SUMMARY Variation of temperature acts asa stimulus. Below the optimum the direct effect of variation of temperature is a retardation of growth, and the negative after-effect, owing to absorption of stimulus, is an acceleration of growth. Hence repeated variations of temperature below the optimum have little resultant effect on growth. Above the optimum, stimulus is not held latent, and there is no negative after-effect of accelerated growth. Hence, repeated variations of temperature above the optimum will retard growth. While rise of temperature up to the optimum accelerates growth, thermal radiation, as such, acts as a stimulus, and retards growth. The unilateral action of thermal radiation is similar to that of luminous radiation—that is to say, thermal radiation, when of moderate intensity, gives rise to positive, and when strong, to neutral, or negative movement. Electric waves induce retardation of growth. The uni- lateral application of electric radiation is also found to induce responsive movements. High frequency Tesla currents retard growth. CH ae hE Rec Vv ON PHOTONASTIC PHENOMENA AND ON DIURNAL SLEEP Photonasty and para-heliotropism—Response of 7vofccolum majus—Responses of plagiotropic stems : (a) AZimosa—(b) [pomwea—(c) Cucurbita—Daily periodic movements of plagiotropic stems—Responsive movements of pulvinated organs —Pulvinated organs showing positive heliotropic movement : (a) Response of terminal leaflet of Desmodium —(b) Response of leaflet of Rodinia—(c) Re- sponsive movements of leaflets of Erythrinaindica and Clhitorta ternatea—The negative heliotropic type of response : (z) Response of pulvinus of JZz0sa— (6) Diurnal sleep of Oxa/is—(c) Diurnal sleep of Azophytum —Directive verses non-directive action of light—General view of responsive curvatures induced in different organs by unilateral application of light. I SHALL now enter upon the investigation of a very large class of phenomena, brought about by the action of light, which have hitherto been regarded not only as obscure, but also as totally unrelated to each other. These phenomena may be described in a general way as the exhibition of the differential effects of light on anisotropic or dorsi-ventral organs. Such effects may again be divided for convenience into two classes, according as they are exhibited either by growing or by mature and pulvinated organs. The respon- sive curvature in the former of these cases, due to the differential growth induced by light, we shall designate as Photonastic. it must be understood that there is in reality, as we shall see, no fundamental difference between the responsive curvatures induced in growing organs, whether radial or dorsi-ventral, and those in pulvinated organs; but it is nevertheless suitable for the purposes of this investigation to treat them under separate headings. Photonasty and para-heliotropism.—In experimenting with dorsi-ventral shoots, De Vries found that when light was applied to the lower or normally shaded surface of, for 622 PLANT RESPONSE example, runners of Lysimachia Nummularia, the result was a concavity of that side ; and when strong light was applied on the upper surface, the result was still the concavity of the normally shaded, and now distal, side. He obtained similar results also with midribs of leaves. Again, in the case of Marchantia thallus, it was found by Frank and Sachs that it was the normally shaded side which became concave, whether light was applied above or below. De Vries explained the curvature away from light, when the dorsal surface was illuminated, by the assumption ‘that the organ was negatively heliotropic ; but the concave curvature induced when the lower surface of the same organ was illuminated necessitates the description of that surface, at least, as positively heliotropic. Thus we are driven to assume that the two surfaces of the organ are endowed at the same time with two opposite heliotropic properties, the obvious impossibility of which has led to the idea that in this class of phenomena we have not to deal with the directive action of light at all. In the case of certain pulvinated organs I shall be able to show an exact parallel to these phenomena—that is to say, an induction under light of concavity in the lower surface, whether light be applied from above or below. This being so, it is clear that the only theory of the phenomena which could be satisfactory would be one which would apply equally to both. Closely connected with the same inquiry is the pheno- menon of para-heliotropism, or the so-called diurnal sleep. Under the intense illumination of midday, the leaves or leaflets of certain plants take up positions which outwardly resemble those which they adopt at night. In some cases again, in which the normal daylight position is outspread, and the nocturnal one of downward folding, the effect of light at midday on the leaflets is to induce a folding upwards. These phenomena have not yet been satisfactorily explained. Darwin suggested that such habits had been acquired for the special purpose of avoiding too intense an illumination. I shall be able, however, to offer a simple and inclusive ex- PHOTONASTIC PHENOMENA AND DIURNAL SLEEP 623 planation, applicable to the phenomena both of photonasty and of para-heliotropism. It will be noticed that in the first case we have to deal with the effect of light on all growing dorsi-ventral organs, and in the second with its effect on mature dorsi-ventral pulvini; and in dealing with both these classes I shall pass step by step from the consideration of simple to that of more complex types. The response of Tropzolum majus.—Sachs found that when the stem of Z7op@olum majus is exposed to intense and long-continued unilateral illumination, a negative helio- tropic curvature is induced ; but when the plant is exposed to moderate unilateral illumination it exhibits positive helio- tropic movement. The explanation of this difference is made quite clear from the experiments which I have already described, with reference to seedlings of Szzapzs nzgra and to the tendril of V7ztzs; for it has been shown that in these cases moderate unilateral stimulus of light caused a positive heliotropic effect ; whereas, under the action of intense stimu- lation, a transient anisotropy was induced, on account of which the excitability of the over-stimulated proximal side was diminished. Hence the distal side was rendered the relatively more excitable, and the intense stimulus becoming internally diffused caused a concavity of the distal side, and gave rise to a negative heliotropic effect (p. 609). The negative effect is thus due to anisotropy, and internal diffusion of excitation from the proximal to the distal sides, this latter factor evidently tending to be facilitated by any agency that increases the conductivity of the tissue (p. 603). We have also seen elsewhere that this power of conductivity is very much augmented in summer (p. 475). In connection with this, it is interesting to note that the negative heliotropic curvature of Zvrop@olum majus, due to transmitted excita- tion, has only been observed by Sachs during summer, while in autumn the effect observed by him was always positive, owing to stimulus remaining localised. Another instance of the same kind is furnished by the hypocotyl of Ivy (Hedera helix), which from the normal positive heliotropic curvature, in 624 PLANT RESPONSE autumn, as observed by Darwin, passes into the exhibition of a strongly negative heliotropic condition in summer. Responses of plagiotropic stems.—It is found that under feeble diffuse illumination the Gourd-plant (Cucurbzta) for example, grows erect. In the open, however, if, on account of its own weight, or by the action of the wind, it is once made to bend, the stem is brought into a position where its upper surface is constantly acted on by strong light, whilst its lower is shaded. By the continuous action of the stimulus of light the upper surface is now rendered less excitable, and a permanent anisotropy (plagiotropism) is induced, such as we saw transiently exhibited in the hypocoty] of Szzapzs (p. 609) under the short experimental exposure of a specimen to intense unilateral illumination. It has already been shown in Chapter VII. that such plagio- tropic stems are more excitable on their lower or shaded side, and that under diffuse stimulation response is by con- cavity of that side. Hence a recumbent plagiotropic stem of this description, acted on by strong vertical light, which becomes internally diffused, will always exhibit concavity of the lower surface, in consequence of which it will be closely pressed against the ground. Such a plagiotropic organ, then, acted upon dorsally, shows a concavity of the ventral surface, or a negative helio- tropic effect ; but if the ventral, or normally shaded, side be itself acted upon directly by light, the result will still be the concavity of the lower or more sensitive surface—that is to say, a positive heliotropic effect. In the former case, then, we have an example of the differential response of an aniso- tropic organ to diffuse stimulation, by concavity of the more excitable ; and in the latter, the direct contraction and con- cavity of the surface acted upon, which happens in this case to be also the more excitable. This will explain why, in the midribs of leaves, in plagiotropic shoots of Lyszmachza, and in the thallus of Warchantia, we always observe the concavity of the shaded and more excitable side, in response to the action of strong light, whether it is applied from above or below. ae PHOTONASTIC PHENOMENA AND DIURNAL SLEEP 625 (@) Mimosa.—This fact, that in plagiotropic stems under the diffuse stimulation of light it is the more excitable shaded side which becomes concave, I have been able to demonstrate by numerous other experiments; for example, taking four creeping stems of J/zimosa, I tied them in such a manner that their free ends should be vertical. The shaded sides of the four specimens were turned so as to face each a different point of the compass —east, west, north, and south. Subjected thus to the diffuse stimulation of light from the sky, they all executed curva- tures. The specimen whose under side faced the east be- came bent towards the east. The same happened to those which faced north, south, and west—that is to say, they became curved towards the north, south, and west re- spectively. The fundamental responsive action by which all these were determined was the induced concavity of Fic. 251. Response to Diurnal Light and Darkness of Plagiotropic Stem the under, or normally shaded, of Zpomea held vertical side, which is the more excit- The curvature is seen to increase with l the progress of day, the shaded side able. to the left becoming more and more (0) T[pomea.— Another ex- concave. The dotted figures repre- . sent positions of gradual recovery ample was that of the creeping Spa stem of /pomaa. This I tied up vertically with its end free, at 10 A.M. the normally shaded side being represented in the diagram to the left (fig. 249). For purposes of the record I placed behind it a piece of paper, on which its different positions were traced from time to time. It will be seen that by 1 P.M it had become considerably curved, the normally shaded side being concave. This concavity had become still more ss 626 PLANT RESPONSE marked by 4 P.M. The next record was taken at 7 P.M., when the sun had gone down, and the source of stimulus was therefore removed. The dotted portion of the record shows the partial recovery which had now taken place, and whose extent was still further increased by Io P.M. This partial undoing of the induced curvature is mainly due to the recovery which is always observed on ces- sation of stimulus. It might, however, be urged that geotropic action, ab- sent when the plant was vertical, and coming into increasing effectiveness with its growing horizon- tality, played a large part in bringing it about. In order, then, to eliminate this ele- ment, I next experimented on a stem of Cucurbita. FIG. 252. Response to Daily Periodic (¢) Cucurbita. — This Light and Darkness of Plagiotropic plagiotropic stem was held Stem of Cucurbita maxima - : ; horizontally and sideways, The specimen held horizontally, with plane, of dorsi-ventral division vertical, the 1 such a Way that the ventral surface being here represented plane which divided its to the right. The action of gravitation is thus practically eliminated. Cumula- dorsal and ventral halves tive effect of day’s illumination is seen i in progressive concavity of normally was vertical. The shaded lower or shaded surface. Dotted figures sjde is here represented in show positions of recovery after dark. ; rf the diagram to the right (fig. 250). Owing to this particular arrangement of the plant it will be seen that the responsive heliotropic move- ment must take place in a horizontal plane, and that on it geotropism will have no influence. The free end of the plagiotropic stem had been naturally somewhat curved up- wards, and this appeared, when placed sideways, as a slight curve to the left, in its position at 7 A.M. Under the stimulus _— ES PHOTONASTIC PHENOMENA AND DIURNAL SLEEP 627 of daylight by 10 A.M. the stem is seen to have taken up a strongly curved position, with concavity of the shaded side. This process is seen from the figure to have been progressive, but on the approach of darkness there was recovery, the return being considerable at 7 P.M., and still greater at 10 P.M. It may be stated here that such recoveries are never quite complete, part of the curvature being fixed by growth. Thus in a naturally growing plagiotropic stem, the portion which was one day slightly lifted above the ground is on the next, owing to this residual effect, made to lie closely against it, so that by the action of light the growing stem is pressed progressively closer to the earth. Daily periodic movement of plagiotropic stem.—From this demonstration of light-curvature and recovery, it will be seen that the free organ has a daily periodicity in virtue of which, in the daytime, it moves gradually downwards, and at night, owing to recovery, upwards. In this plagiotropic stem, then, we see, as will be shown later, the first induction of that nyctitropic movement which is more strikingly dis- played in dorsi-ventral leaves. Responsive movement of pulvinated organs.—The demonstration which I have just made of the peculiar responses of anisotropic organs to the stimulus of light, as exemplified in the case of plagiotropic stems, will be found still more strikingly applicable to pronouncedly dorsi-ventral organs like pulvini. In the last-named instance, indeed, as motility is great, the investigation has the advantage of con- cerning itself with responsive effects which take place very quickly. From what has already been said, we shall be prepared to meet with. two different types of responses, according as the transverse conductivity of the organ is feeble or consider- able. In the former of these cases, under unilateral stimu- lation, there is no internal diffusion of stimulus, and the side acted on, whether above or below, will respond by concavity ; but in the latter case—that is to say, where the transverse conductivity of the tissue is great—it will be the more excit- sS2 628 PLANT RESPONSE able side which, under strong unilateral stimulation of either, will become concave. Supposing the lower side to be the more excitable, then, strong stimulation, whether of dorsal or of ventral, will bring about concavity of the lower; but here we must bear in mind the possibility of an effect which will be the result of moderate stimulus. The differential effect, which brings about the concavity of the lower, even when it is the dorsal surface that is excited, depends on the internal diffusion of stimulation, and this in turn is dependent not only on the conductivity of the tissue, but also on the intensity of the stimulus. If, then, a feeble or moderate stimulus be applied on the dorsal surface, even of a highly conducting organ, the result will be a concavity of that surface. The long-continued action of moderate stimu- lus will, however, bring about a gradual percolation, and the first response due to localised, will give place to the differential effect of diffuse, stimulus. If the stimulus again be very much stronger, this reversal will take place much more quickly. We thus see that responsive movements may be positive, neutral, or negative, according to the strength and duration of the stimulus. Pulvinated organ showing positive heliotropic move- ment: (a) Terminal leaflet of Desmodium.—We shall now take the case of a pulvinus in which the conductivity is feeble, and in which we should therefore expect to obtain positive response. I have already given response-curves in illustration of this positive response, whether it be the upper or lower surface of the pulvinus which is subjected to light (p. 586). It was also shown that the leaflet moved towards the light, and that in such cases it was the pulvinus, and not the lamina, which was both the perceptive and responding region. (6) Response of leaflet of Robinta.—The leaflets of Rodznza, under the vertical light of the noonday sun, exhibit what is known as diurnal sleep—that is to say, they fold themselves upwards. This is simply an instance of the positive helio- tropic effect common to all those pulvinated organs in which PHOTONASTIC PHENOMENA AND DIURNAL SLEEP 629 transverse conductivity is feeble. I give here (fig. 253) a response-record obtained with a leaflet of Rodzuza under the action of sunlight from above. A similar response, but in a downward direction, is obtained when light is made to act from below ; but as, under natural conditions, light always acts from above, the leaflets are found, when the acting daylight is sufficiently strong, to exhibit what is known as diurnal sleep, or the para-heliotropic effect, in an upward direction. I have obtained numerous similar records fic. 253. Positive Helio- with other leaflets, which fold upwards rope" p\sponee ot under the action of sunlight. Acting from Above (c) Responsive movements of leaflets Dotted line represents _re- of Erythrina indica and of Chtoria rie ae eee ternatea.—For the sake of simplicity represent intervals of five I described the movement of Rodbznza yas leaflet as upward; but the actual direction-is one which more or less accurately coincides with that of incident sunlight. As further examples of this particular type of diurnal sleep movement, I may mention the leaflets of Erythrina tndica and Chitorta ternatea (Indian name Afarajita). Both of these are so remarkably sensitive that they follow the course of the sun, in such a way that the axis of the cup formed by the folding leaflets at the end of the petiole is coincident with Fic. 254. Positive Helio- : : 2 tropic Response of Leaf- the rays of light, and continues so lets OF Beaune aeiica foimabout 11 A.M. till about 3 P.M. The leaflets form a hollow The negative heliotropic type cup with direction _ of incident sunlight as axis. of response.—We shall now pass on to the second, or negative, heliotropic type of response due to the internal diffusion of stimulus; and in order to show that there is a continuity between these and the 630 PLANT RESPONSE former instances, I shall here refer once more to the case, already mentioned, of the heliotropic response of the terminal leaflet of Desmodium, which in winter is always positive, but in a given experiment, under the conditions of greater transverse conductivity which are brought on in summer, exhibited, after two hours of continuous exposure to vertical sunlight, a neutralisation of the previous positive effect (p. 604). (a) Response of pulvinus of Mimosa.—The second type of response will be the better understood if we first study in detail all its characteristics. As I have already said, these negative heliotropic responses result from the transverse diffusion of stimulus across the tissue, which brings about the concavity of the more excitable lower half of the organ. We can easily, in the continuous response-record to be given presently, detect this gradual process of the percolation of stimulus through the tissue, when light is applied from above. As there is in the case of J/zmosa a considerable mass of inter- vening tissue between the upper and lower surfaces of the pulvinus, it follows that unless the stimulus applied be excessive, there will be a certain interval of time required for its passage. We should therefore expect that on the application of light to the dorsal surface there would be a local contraction and concavity of that side, raising the leaf up, and causing a preliminary positive response; but this movement will be arrested, and gradually reversed, so soon as the stimulus reaches the lower side, and begins to induce antagonistic contraction there; and after this, the greater excitability of the lower will be manifested by its greater contraction, as seen in the negative response, or depression of the leaf. All this will be clearly understood from the series of records given below. It is necessary, however, before describing the effect of the continuous application of light from above, to analyse the responsive sensibilities of the two sides of this organ ; and this is the more desirable since, in the case of MWzmosa, it is commonly assumed that excitability characterises only ~~ a PHOTONASTIC PHENOMENA AND DIURNAL SLEEP 63 the lower half of the organ, the responsive movement of the pulvinus being due to that factor alone. In the course of the present work, however, I have frequently stated that the upper half also was excitable, and that the usual responsive movement was due to the adzfferential excitability of the two. It is now easy, using localised stimulus of light, to submit this question to a crucial test. First I took a record of the responsive movement of the leaf of Mzmosa, when the upper half of the pulvinus alone was subjected to stimulus of sunlight. Fig. 255, a, shows the moderate positive, or in this case upward, movement, which was the result of this sti- mulation. By means of a properly inclined mirror light was now thrown vertically up- wards, so as to strike the lower half of the pulvinus. The con- sequent positive re- Fic. 255. Responses of M/mosa to Sunlight of sponsive movement, not too long Duration in this case down. (a) Light acting on pulvinus from above ; (é) light wards, is seen to be acting on pulvinus from below ; (c) light acting simultaneously from above and below. Dotted much stronger (fig. line represents recovery on cessation of light. 255, 0), on account of the greater excitability of the lower half of the organ. The differential character of the responsive movement under externally diffused stimulation is shown in fig. 255, c, which is a record of the response given by the pulvinus when both its upper and lower sides were simultaneously acted upon by light. It will be seen that in this record, then, we have a case of response to externally diffused stimulus. We shall next observe the effect of stimulus which has become zxzernally diffused, owing to conduction from the upper to the lower 632 PLANT RESPONSE half of the pulvinus. We have just seen that in the case of externally diffused stimulation both sides of the organ are acted upon at the same moment, and the differential response is downward from the beginning; but when continuous stimulus is applied on the dorsal surface, it is at first unilateral, and only afterwards becomes internally diffused. We therefore obtain in this case (fig. 256), as was theoretically inferred, all the phases of re- sponse, positive, passing through neutral, into negative. A very interesting feature of this record is the after-effect, on the cessation of stimulus, which is represented by a dotted line. In Chapter XXXIV., while deal- ing with the detection of the Fic. 256. Response of Pulvinus latent factor, it was explained of Aimosa to Action of Con- that there are two distinct after- tinuous Light from Above ap- et ; plied at Moment marked with | effects, positive and negative Positive heliotropic | movement (p. AST ). In the former, the caused by excitation of upper Soo Ae tuks : half neutralised by transmission movement 1s simply a continua- to distal side, and ultimately tjon of the effect seen when reversed owing to greater ex- : i citability of lower half. Dotted stimulus was acting; but the line represents recovery on ces- latter, or negative, after-effect, sation of light. Note final , erection of leaf above original being due to the increase of by absorbed stimulus, ‘latent energy by absorption of incident stimulus, finds expres- sion in an opposite movement. In the case of the response of growth we saw that the positive after-effect consisted of a persistent retardation, and the negative of an acceleration, of growth. In the case of pulvinated organs, however, the negative after effect is generally indistinguishable from the movement of recovery ; but in the present instance we find it clearly exhibited in the fact that the after-effect not only causes recovery to the original position, but carries the leaf to a distance beyond. The positive heliotropic response of the leaf, then, persists PHOTONASTIC PHENOMENA AND DIURNAL SLEEP 633 only as long as it takes the stimulus to reach the distal side of the pulvinus. When strong concentrated sunlight is applied from above, therefore, the duration of this upward movement is reduced, and it appears only as a slight twitch. The same effect is produced under less intense light when, as in thinner pulvinated organs, the distance to be traversed by stimulus is not so great. (6) The diurnal sleep of Oxalis—In consequence of strong vertical illumination, as at noonday, negative heliotropic response, with downward folding of the leaves, takes place in those pulvinated organs in which there is internal diffusion of sti- mulus, and in which the lower side of the pulvinus is much more excitable than the upper. As ex- amples of this, we shall take the cases of Oxvalis and Lzophytum. In the former, if light of moderate intensity, say from alamp, Fic. 257. Responses of Oxa/z?s to Sun- light (a) Shows greater excitation of lower half be applied on the dorsal and downward response when both upper and lower are simultaneously or upper surface, an up- subjected to stimulus of light ; (4) shows ward, or positive heliotro- downward or negative response owing ‘ Ee iate to internal diffusion of stimulus when pic curvature is induced. upper surface only is acted on by : strong light. Arrows indicate the di- If the same light be rections of incident light. applied below, a positive heliotropic movement, in this case downwards, is again induced; but the fact that the lower side is much the more excitable is seen when the upper and lower surfaces are excited simultaneously, for there is now a downward movement (fig. 257, a). Thus external diffuse stimulation induces downward movement, and we shall also find that stimulus internally diffused has the same effect. This is seen by throwing a strong beam of sunlight on the upper surface, 634 PLANT RESPONSE when the leaflet is found to respond by depression (fig. 255, 2 ). Here, then, we have a repetition of those movements which we have already seen in the case of plagiotropic stems, where strong illumination of whichever surface always had the same effect, that is to say the induced concavity in the lower. (c) Diurnal sleep of Biophytum—tIn the case of Oxals and in that of J/zmosa the responsive movement is more or less continuous; but in such a pronouncedly multiply- responding organ as that of Bzophytum, the effect of internally diffused stimulus on the more excitable lower half of the pulvinus is to induce depression by a series of multiple responses. This is well seen in the following record (fig. 258), where under the action of strong sunlight from above the leaflet is undergoing a progressive fall. A record of a similar effect in the case of Biophytum is seen in fig. — 123, the fall being in that case represented by an up curve. The specimen there was somewhat sluggish, and there were three pulsations in the course of ten Fic. 258 Negative Multiple minutes, making an average of 3°3 Response in Arophytum minutes to each. In the present when Acted on from Above , : by Strong Sunlight instance, however, there were six pulsations in the course of fifteen minutes—each, that is to say, having an average period of 2°5 minutes. Similarly in Averrhoa the fall of the leaflet occurs in a pulsating manner, an instance of which is given by Darwin.' The directive versus non-directive action of light.—It will be well at this point to enter into the question of arbitrary distinction which is commonly made as between heliotropic action in radial organs and the same action in the anisotropic or dorsi-ventral. It is clear, however, that such a distinction ' Darwin, AZovements of Plants, p. 333: PHOTONASTIC PHENOMENA AND DIURNAL SLEEP 635 is no longer possible when the fundamental unity of effects in the two cases has been perceived. But it is customary to make a further distinction between these effects, on the ground that the movement of the radial is determined by, while that of the anisotropic is independent of, the directive action of light. In this matter again I shall be able to show further that no such line of demarcation can be drawn. It will then be seen that the differences between the two classes of phenomena are only apparent. In the case of radial organs, we saw that the direct action of moderate stimulus of light, inducing concavity of the proximal, gave rise to positive heliotropic movement. The same is true of the similar direct action of a moderate intensity of light on, say, the pulvinus of JZzmosa, where the upper and lower halves of the organ both exhibit positive response. With stronger light, again, the radial organ develops an induced anisotropy, by which the proximal side, owing to fatigue, becomes the relatively less excitable. Under this condition, the proximal side of the radial organ corresponds to the less excitable upper half of the pulvinus of Mimosa, and the distal to the more excitable lower half. This differentiation is seen to be continuous throughout the three cases of the radial organ, the plagiotropic stem, and the pulvinus, although in the first of these it is transient, lasting only during the action of stimulus. In all these cases alike, strong unilateral stimulus, acting on the less excitable proximal or upper surface, becomes internally diffused, and causes movement away from stimulus, or negative heliotropic response, in a direction which is perpendicular to the plane of separation of the two anisotropic halves. Thus there is no difference between the negative heliotropic responses of a radial and a pulvinated organ. The distinction between the two cases of the permanent and temporary differentiation of the organ has been illustrated by Sachs by a reference to the induction of polarity in steel and in soft iron respectively. According to this analogy, Marchantia behaves to intense light like steel to a magnet, 636 PLANT RESPONSE retaining permanently the induced polarity. Tvrope@olum majus, on the other hand, behaves like soft iron, assuming a definite but temporary polarity, which disappears when the influence of light, like that of the magnet, is withdrawn. This illustration, however, though vivid, is likely to be misleading, for it suggests that, under strong stimulus, the normal pro- perties of the organ pass into a polar or opposite condition. But there is no such change. Light merely induces a difference of excitability in the two sides. That there is no reversal of heliotropic sensibility is shown by the fact that either side, when excited by moderate intensity of stimulus, gives positive response. The necessary condition for the exhibition of negative heliotropic response is not only the differential excitability of the two sides of the organ, but also the internal diffusion of stimulus. Owing, however, to the permanence of this differentiation in a pulvinated organ, there will be certain conditions under which the action of light will appear to be non-directive, for it is the diffuse stimulus, however produced, that brings about the responsive concavity of the more excitable lower half of the pulvinus, and this internal diffusion will take place just the same, whatever be the flank of the organ on which the strong external stimulus may have been applied. In the case of the radial organ, on the other hand, the differentiation between the less excitable proximal and more excitable distal is not fixed, but changes according to the side acted upon at the time by light. The different responsive movements induced by light— positive, negative, dia-heliotropic, and para-heliotropic—have hitherto been ascribed, as I have already had occasion to point out, to as many specific sensibilities possessed by the plant. They have now, however, been demonstrated to be but so many examples of the general law that plant-organs respond to stimulus, by the induced concavity of the relatively more excited. There is thus no fundamental discontinuity between the responses of radial and of dorsi-ventral organs, or between PHOTONASTIC PHENOMENA AND DIURNAL SLEEP 637 positive heliotropic and negative heliotropic response. This will be seen still more clearly in the following statement, applying to the two extreme cases of radial and of dorsi- ventral organs, the plagiotropic constituting an intermediate link between the two. General view of responsive curvatures induced in different organs by unilateral application of light 1. Radialorgans : (a) Posztive response : stimulus localised, the proximal side more excited and concave; eg. Sznapis under moderate unilateral light (p. 609). (6) Intermediate or neutral response: stimulus internally diffused, causing equal excitation of proximal and distal sides ; the organ takes up a neutral (so-called dia-heliotropic) posi- tion, at right angles to light; e.g. Szzapzs under moderately strong unilateral light (p. 609); certain tendrils apparently insensitive. (c) Negative response : strong stimulus internally diffused, which also induces anisotropy, the distal being then the more excitable ; concavity of the more excited distal ; ¢,g¢. seedling of Sinapts, and tendril of Vz¢2s under strong unilateral sunlight (pp. 609, 611). 2. Dorsi-ventral organs: (a) Postive response: stimulus localised by relative non-conductivity of organ: the more excited proximal side concave; eg. positive movement of terminal leaflet of Desmodium ; the diurnal sleep movements of Robinia, Erythrina indica, and Clitorta ternatea (p. 629). (6) L[ntermediate or neutral response: stimulus internally diffused; upper and lower halves equally excited; eg. terminal leaflet of Desmodium, under several hours of vertically acting sunlight (p. 604). (c) Negative response: strong stimulus acting from above on highly conducting organ, of which lower half is con- siderably more excitable ; concavity of the more excitable lower half ; ¢.g. fall of I/cmosa leaf under strong illumination from above ; diurnal sleep movements of Oxales, Lrophytum, and Averrhoa. 638 PLANT RESPONSE SUMMARY The responsive action of growing organs being not essentially different from that of pulvinated organs, a similar explanation must be applicable to the two classes of phenomena. Owing tolong-continued unilateral excitation by light, the upper side of a plagiotropic stem is rendered the relatively less excitable. Hence such organs may be regarded as equivalent to diffuse pulvinoids, et which the upper side is the less excitable. In both these cases, of pulvini and aiffene pulvinoids, the local application of moderate stimulus on either side induces normal positive response. But with long-continued application of strong stimulus two types of response may be obtained, according as the organ is characterised by a feeble or high power of trans- verse conductivity. In the former of these cases the stimu- lus will remain localised, and the response will be positive. In the second, the stimulus will become internally diffused, and if the lower side be the more excitable, stimulation of the upper will give rise to the concavity of the lower, con- stituting negative response. As examples of the first of these classes may be mentioned the diurnal sleep movements of Robinza, Erythrina indica, and Clitorta ternatea, in which vertical illumination induces upward folding of the leaflets. As representing the second class, we have both hb Ree tropic and pulvinated organs. The plagiotropic stems of M/zmosa, Ipomaa, and Cucur- dita exhibit increasing concavity of the lower side with the duration of the day’s illumination. A periodic downward movement is thus induced in them, which reaches its maximum at the end of the day. The reverse movement, of gradual erection, occurs during the night. In pulvinated organs, negative heliotropic response, with PHOTONASTIC PHENOMENA AND DIURNAL SLEEP 639 downward folding of the leaflets, is exhibited in the diurnal sleep movements of Ovalzs and Bzophytum. In Mimosa the application of moderate intensity of light from above induces upward or positive heliotropic move- ment. Strong and long-continued application of light on the same upper surface, however, owing to the internal diffusion of excitation, induces concavity of the lower and more excitable side, or negative heliotropic movement. Both these responses may be regarded as cases of the directive action of light; but if the organ be excited by stimulus which is externally diffuse, the responsive movement is still downwards. The directive action of light has thus passed into non-directive. CHAPTER: Syi ON DIA-HELIOTROPISM AND DIA-GEOTROPISM Difficulty of distinguishing between effect of light and other reactions —Theories of Frank and De Vries—Subsidiary factors: (1) Epinasty and hyponasty ; (2) Effect of gravity; (3) Effect of suctional activity and of turgescence ; (4) Modification of effect by characteristic limits of flexibility—Discrimination of the part played by heliotropism in the movement of the leaf—Proof of absence of any specific dia-heliotropic tendency in leaves—The lamina not the perceptive organ—Principal types of the response of leaves to stimulus of light—Positive type of response: mango leaf—Negative type of response : leaf of Artocarpus. WE shall now take up the question of the effects of illumination on ordinary leaves, which are generaily supposed, on account of some special dia-heliotropic sensibility, to have the habit of placing themselves at right angles to incident light. We have seen in the last chapter that the movement of anisotropic organs under heliotropic stimulation is not the result of any specific sensitiveness, but constitutes a simple instance of the response of plant-organs to all forms of stimulus, the response being appropriately modified in this particular case by the anatomical and physiological pecu- liarities of the responding organ. We have been able to demonstrate the continuity of this responsive phenomenon by analysing it in two extreme cases of the anisotropic differentiation, those namely of the plagiotropic stem, in which we see one of its earlier phases, and of dorsi-ventral pulvinated organs, in which it attains its highest development. In the movements of ordinary leaves we have what is merely an intermediate stage between these, hence any explanation which would elucidate their action must be one which is applicable to both the extremes. tee ri DIA-HELIOTROPISM AND DIA-GEOTROPISM 641 Difficulty of distinguishing between effect of light and other reactions.—In the case of the response of pulvinated organs, we had the advantage, owing to their great motility, of being able to refer each particular responsive movement to the immediate stimulating action of incident light. In ordinary leaves, however, the movements being very sluggish, it takes so long a time for any given responsive action to attain the requisite magnitude for ordinary observation, that other factors of variation intervene, and it becomes difficult to know how much of the resultant response is due to heliotropic stimulus as such. It is this great difficulty of disentangling the response due to light, which is the proper subject of the inquiry, from numerous other subsidiary factors that has led to the existing divergence of views among observers. In the course of the present chapter, therefore, I shall shortly enumerate those various agencies which are subsidiarily instrumental in bringing about the ultimate position assumed by the leaf. I shall then describe a method by which helio- tropic action proper can be discriminated with certainty from other influences. The relation between the fundamental action in response to light, which has already been demonstrated, and the heliotropic response of the leaves will thus be made apparent. But before entering upon these questions I shall briefly allude to the principal existing theories on this subject. Theory of Frank.—There are at present two main types of opinion with regard to the question of the effect of light on leaves. Frank and his school account for that action of leaves by which they place themselves with their flat surfaces perpen- dicular to the direction of incidence of the rays of light, or of the action of gravity, by assuming that dorsi-ventral organs possess a peculiar property of sensitiveness to the directive action of light and gravity. This they designate as Transverse or Dia-heliotropism, and Transverse or Dia-geotropism. It is supposed that the habit has been acquired for the advantage of the plant, inasmuch as the leaves, by placing themselves feed by 642 PLANT RESPONSE in this particular position, are enabled to absorb the largest amount of sunlight. Such arguments, however, do not throw any light on the mechanism by which the movement is brought about. It is even somewhat difficult to understand how this generalisation, that leaves place themselves at right angles to light, has come to be accepted as a universal fact ; for it is only necessary to make a visit to the open forest in order to see that, so far from this being the case, many leaves place themselves vertically. upwards, others downwards, and the rest in all possible intermediate angles between the two. All that can be claimed on behalf of the dia-heliotropic position is that no deviation from it is greater than plus or minus QO. Theory of De Vries.—De Vries, however, is in disagree- ment with this theory. He establishes the importance of the unequal rates of growth in the upper and lower halves of the leaves—z.e. epinasty or hyponasty—as a factor in bringing about their ultimate attitude. He next assumes, in accordance with the generally accepted view, the existence of two opposite responsive reactions, positive and negative, in regard to light and gravity. He then proceeds to show that there can be no necessity for the further assumption of a third or dia-heliotropic tendency in the leaves, since epinasty and hyponasty, their different combinations with either of the opposite actions of positive and negative heliotropism and geotropism, and considerations of the weight and balance of parts, are all factors which take a share in determining the position ulti- mately to be assumed by the dorsi-ventral organ with regard to light. I shall now attempt to demonstrate the fact that the responses of ordinary leaves are in every way similar to those of the pulvinated, the mechanics of whose movements” have already been fully described. It will further be shown how and under what circumstances the normal positive passes into negative response, through certain intermediate phases. Subsidiary factors.— But before undertaking either of these inquiries, it will be well to enumerate briefly the DIA-HELIOTROPISM AND DIA-GEOTROPISM 643 subsidiary factors which combine with the heliotropic action proper in determining the final position of the leaf. 1. Epinasty and hyponasty—These unequal growths of one side or the other, De Vries believed to be brought about by some spontaneous unknown cause in the plant itself. Detmer, however, came to the conclusion that, as regarded epinasty, it was not spontaneous, but induced by the action of light. This conclusion was based on the observations (1) that cotyledons of Cwcurbita remained closed up, in continuous darkness, but opened out when subjected to light ; and (2) that the primordial leaves of Phaseolus, kept in darkness, remained folded, and only opened out on illumina- tion. The investigations of Vines, however, though he supports the contention of Detmer with regard to Phaseolus, have led him to a different view in the case of Cucurbita, where he finds the epinastic movement to be induced even in the absence of light. He has therefore come to the conclusion that the phenomena of epinasty and hyponasty are spontaneous, and not directly due to the action of illumination. These differences of opinion, however, have arisen from the obscurity in which autonomous or spontaneous movement has been involved, and they may be expected to disappear when its true nature is clearly perceived. These alternate movements of growth are in fact only another example of multiple or autonomous response, the difference between them and those other forms with which we are already acquainted lying in the greater slowness of their period, and in the relative fewness of the pulsations that can be exhibited. In the case of autonomous pulsation or circumnutation of stems, since the growth on which they depend is indefinitely prolonged, we have an indefinite continuance of these pulsa- tions. In the case of leaves, however, the organ usually _ completes only half its swing, whether epinastic or hyponastic, and by that time further pulsation is arrested, owing to the cessation of growth; yet in some cases there is more than a semi-pulsation, as where hyponastic movement is followed TT 2 644 PLANT RESPONSE by epinastic. It has already been shown that autonomous response in general can take place only when the internal energy or the sum total of the latent stimulating factors is above par. This is equally true of the autonomous response of growth itself. In the case of autonomous epi- or hypo- nastic pulsations, therefore, a certain amount of internal energy is essential for their initiation. This is in some cases supplied by other forms of stimulus, but there may be others in which light is the critical factor. In order, then, to study the directive action of light on dorsi-ventral organs under normal tonic conditions, we must be able to determine the characteristic influence which will be exerted by the stimulus of incident light in modification of the already existing movement of the organ. This inquiry is therefore exactly parallel to our previous study of the action of light in modifying the existing growth-movements of radial organs. In that case the vartations induced in the ordinary rate of movement afforded us a measure of the effect of the stimulus of light. In the case of dorsi-ventral organs such as leaves, similarly, the effect of light can be correctly inferred only by observing the variations which it induces in their existing movements. The manner in which this is done will be described presently. © ; 2. Effect of gravity—tIn long-continued experiments on the curvatures induced by light, the observed movement is also modified in part by the influence of geotropism. This geotropic action in leaves is by some investigators believed to be of two types, negative and positive. Others, again, regard it as dia-geotropic. I shall, however, adduce con- siderations which will show that the upper and lower halves of dorsi-ventral organs exhibit differential excitability to geo- tropic as to other forms of stimulus. In order to neutralise the geotropic action, and thus study the heliotropic effect alone, Francis Darwin mounted the plant on a rotating klinostat. It is true that in a strictly radial organ the geotropic effect is successfully neutralised by rotation on the klinostat, since in this case the geotropic sensitiveness of the DIA-HELIOTROPISM AND DIA-GEOTROPISM 645 different flanks is the same; but this, as will appear from certain experiments to be described later, cannot be said of dorsi-ventral organs, for in these geotropic sensitiveness is different in the upper and lower halves. In my own experi- ments on the action of light, however, I shall be able to give results which are but little affected by geotropism. 3. Effect of suctional activity and of turgescence—There are two other factors not hitherto taken into account which exert considerable influence in determining the attitude finally assumed by the leaf. These are the general condition of turgescence of the plant, and the limits of flexibility which characterise the particular responding organ. The first of these is, as we have seen, dependent on the suctional activity. Now, the mechanical response of a plant-organ is the result, as we already know, of the expulsion of water from the excited tissue; but if the tissue be over-turgid expulsion of water is opposed, and the responsive movement is thereby reduced or abolished, as we have seen in the case of Mimosa in a condition of excessive turgor. The same phenomenon I have again seen manifested, in a remarkable manner, in the difference of the movements made by the leaves in response to light according as they were normally- or super- turgid. Thus, in a small plant of Artocarpus, grown in a pot, and in the autumn season, when the suctional activity was not great, the leaves responded to light by making a progressive angle with the vertical, so that, under the long-continued action of light, they first reached the horizontal position and then fell many degrees below it ; but in the rainy season, when they held themselves abruptly vertical in consequence of excessive turgor, the action of light produced little or no responsive movement. In the autumn season, again, the limited system of roots and rootlets possessed by this plant, when grown in a pot, allows it only a moderate degree of turgescence, and in this condition it readily responds to light; but a large tree of the same species, growing in the open and possessed of a highly ramified and extensive root-system, will during the same 646 PLANT RESPONSE season, its turgescence being great, maintain its leaves ina vertical position, but little affected by the action of light. From this it will also become clear that any influence, such as long maintenance under darkness, which modifies the suctional activity, is liable to render the response of the plant abnormal. 4. Modification of effect by characteristic limits of flexi- b:lity.—I shail here deal with another important factor in the determination of the final position assumed by the leaf—that is to say, with the anatomical peculiarities which determine the limits of flexibility. Let us take the petiole bearing the terminal leaflet of a leaf of Desmodium. We now suppose this leaflet to be outspread, in such a position that its midrib is in a continuous straight line with the petiole. This upper line we shall know as the dorsal line. It consists of two parts or components, the laminal and the petiolar. In this particular position they form a straight line ; but the movement of the leaflet takes place with the point of junction as the hinge. We shall then distinguish that particular position of the dorsal line in which the laminal and petiolar halves are continuous and straight as neutral, and angular movements above or below will be measured accordingly. In the case of Desmodium, when the terminal leaflet has reached the neutral position, it cannot, owing to the anatomical peculiarity of the joint, be bent further up- wards ; but it can be bent in the opposite direction, that is downwards, until the leaflet lies along the under side of the petiole, the curvature being then through 180°. Thus the limits of flexibility of this leaflet may be expressed by the ° formula a3 that is to say, its flexibility above the neutral position is 0°, and below, 180°. Now, the petiole itself, in the case of Desmodium, for example, may, and does, occupy many different positions with regard to the stem. Let us suppose it at a given moment to subtend an acute angle, being thus more or less vertical, and suppose the terminal leaflet to be horizontal. If light now acts from DIA-HELIOTROPISM AND DIA-GEOTROPISM 647 above, the leaflet will move continuously upwards till the lamina has reached the neutral position —that is to say, till the midrib constitutes a straight prolongation of the petiole ; but if light act on the leaflet from below, it will bend downwards (fig. 259, a). If the petiole, however, at the beginning of the observation be horizontal instead of vertical, and the lamina be in the neutral position, then vertical light cannot, owing to the anatomical peculiarities of the pulvinar joint, carry the leaflet further above the dorsal line (fig. 259, 0). Or we may ~e-ees a Fic, 259. Different Limits of Flexibility Vertical light on terminal leaflet of Desmodium causes (a) up movement till the dorsal line is a continuous straight line; light applied below causes movement downwards below this neutral line through 180°. In 4 is shown neutral position, after reaching which there is no further movement upwards. In c is shown movement of terminal leaflet of Erythrina indica wpwards, under vertical illumination, through 180° above the neutral line. again take as an example the terminal leaflet of Erythrina indica (fig. 259, c). The limit of flexibility is in this case represented by an angle of almost 180° above the neutral, whereas downwards its limit is about 90°. The formula is 180° thus - Now, when this leaflet is acted upon by light from above, it may become almost doubled upon the petiole upwards, just as we found the terminal leaflet of Desmodium to be almost doubled downwards. Hence we see that though the heliotropic effect of light is always the same, yet 648 PLANT RESPONSE the position of the leaflet in space--that is to say, its relation to the vertical line—is largely modified not only by the limits which the anatomical structure of the laminal pulvinus or pulvinoid imposes upon its flexibility, but also by the angle which the petiole makes with the stem; for there is, generally speaking, a second pulvinus or pulvinoid at the junction of the petiole and the stem, and this petiolar pulvinus is, in its turn, more or less sensitive to stimulation (p. 59). We have thus obtained some idea of the anatomical elements, regarding the petiole and its joints, which enter into the complex question of the final position assumed by the leaf in space. Discrimination of the part played by heliotropism in the movement of the leaf.—In studying the heliotropic effect we are concerned only with the action of light itself, and not with the resultant effect, due to various co-operating ~ factors. When the action of each of these is definitely understood, it becomes a simple problem to understand the effects due to their combination. The difficulty of this investigation has hitherto lain, as already said, in the fact that, owing to the generally sluggish nature of respon- sive movements in ordinary leaves, a long time must be allowed to elapse before they become measurable, and during this long period other factors may become operative in unknown ways. The effect of light, however, can easily be discriminated by the use of the Optic Lever for record, for this allows us any degree of magnification which may be desired. Thus the natural curve gives us the resultant effect of all the pre-existing factors under normal tonic conditions, and its subsequent variations under the incidence of light at once exhibits the distinctive action of that stimulus. On the withdrawal of light, again, the recovery from the induced variation affords an additional corroboration of the inference that the variation itself had been due to the action of light. In consequence, moreover, of the delicacy of this means of continuous record, the characteristic effect can be detected in the course of a few minutes, thus eliminating the unknown DIA-HELIOTROPISM AND DIA-GEOTROPISM 649 changes which are liable to occur during the lapse of long intervals of time. The action of light is, generally speaking, so predominant over that of the subsidiary factors, that a high magnification of record is not necessary. Under these circumstances, that is to say under low magnification, the record before the application of light is practically a hort- zontal line. Under vertical illumination of the upper side of the leaf, then, deviation above this horizontal represents a positive heliotropic movement, while deflection in the opposite direction signifies the negative. But before I proceed to give details of my experi- ments, I shall adduce facts which will show that there is no inherent tendency in the leaves to move in such a manner as to place themselves at right angles to the light. Proof of absence of any specific dia-heliotropic ten- dency in leaves.—A Des- modium plant was taken, and the petiole fixed horizontally, Fic. 261. Curve showing Time- relations of the Responsive Angular Movement of Terminal Leaflet of Desmodium under Light, as represented in the last figure The original angle of 45° between Fic, 260. Movement of Ter- the surface of the leaflet and the minal Leaflet of Desmodium direction of incident light was placing itself Parallel to reduced to 13° in the course ot Incident Horizontal Light 25 minutes. the terminal leaflet being at an angle of 45° with the horizon. Sunlight was then made to strike it horizontally (fig. 260). It now the leaflet had a dia-heliotropic tendency, it is clear that its movement would be such as to increase its angle with the horizon to 90°, thus bringing the light to strike it at right 650 PLANT RESPONSE angles. Movement in the other direction—that is to say, towards the decrease of the angle—would, on the other hand, indicate that the effect of light was to induce the normal positive heliotropic response. From the record (fig. 261) it will be seen that the latter was the case—that is to say, the leaflet moved continuously, tending to become parallel to the light, having in the course of twenty-five minutes moved from its position at an angle of 45° to one at 13° to the horizon—that is to say, at an average rate of angular movement of about 1°3° per minute. The lamina not the perceptive organ.—If, again, the object of the so-called dia-heliotropic movement had been the absorption by the upper surface of the lamina of the largest possible amount of light, it would have been neces- sary for the lamina to be the perceptive organ, determining its movement according to the direction of stimulus; but that this is not the case may be demonstrated by subjecting the lamina alone to the action of light, and covering the pulvinus with a small opaque shield of black paper. On doing so all movement is found to be arrested. If now, on the other hand, the leaflet be covered with opaque paper, and the pulvinus be left exposed, the usual heliotropic movement is found to take place. This conclusively proves that, as regards the response of the leaf to light, the lamina is not the perceptive organ, and therefore that a supposed advantage to itself cannot be the important factor in determining its move- ment. That the lamina is not the perceptive organ has indeed been shown already in the case of ordinary leaves, when subjected to forms of stimulus other than light (p. 60). We saw this, for example, in the case of a leaf of Artocarpus when subjected to electrical and thermal stimulation. It was in that case shown that since the lamina consisted of a mass of non-conducting parenchymatous tissue, local excitation might be caused by the incidence of stimulus, but could not be effectively transmitted to the distant pulvinus or pulvinoid by which the movement of the leaf was brought about. The only case in which such transmission is possible to some DIA-HELIOTROPISM AND DIA-GEOTROPISM 651 extent in leaves is in the parallel-veined leaves of monocoty- ledons. This fact, that the lamina is not in general the per- ceptive organ with regard to stimulus, say of light, is still further demonstrated in the following experiment, performed on the leaflet of Avythrina indica. Sunlight was first applied locally to the lamina of this leaflet by means of a suitably inclined mirror, at the point in the record which is marked x (fig. 262). It will be noticed that during ten minutes of such application not the slightest responsive movement was in- duced. Keeping everything else the same, the light was Fic. 262. Record showing that Lamina is not the Perceptive Organ Light applied vertically at x on lamina of Zrythrina indica, and con- tinued for ten minutes, produces no response; but when applied on the pulvinus at the arrow (f) there is immediate positive response. Dotted line shows positive after-effect and recovery on cessation of light. now shifted by a movement of the mirror, and thrown directly on the pulvinus, at the point in the record which is marked with an arrow (t). It will be observed that the leaf began to respond immediately by a movement towards the light, and in the course of seventeen minutes’ exposure its tip moved through a distance of 20 mm. or at an average rate of a little over I mm. per minute. On the withdrawal of light the movement persisted, owing to the positive after- effect, for a period of seven minutes, after which recovery began. The marked difference between the quiescent con- 652 PLANT RESPONSE dition of the leaflet during the ten minutes’ exposure of the lamina to the action of light, and its energetic movement immediately on the application of light to the pulvinus, shows once more, in a striking manner, that it is the latter organ and not the lamina whose perception of light is effec- tive in initiating the responsive action. Principal types of the response of leaves to stimulus of light.—I shall now proceed to show that the directive effect of light on leaves is very definite. In studying the response of anisotropic organs—that is to say, plagiotropic shoots and dorsi-ventral pulvinated leaves—we have seen that there are two extreme types of response, of which the first is exhibited by organs possessing feeble transverse con- ductivity, and the second by those in which the transverse conductivity is great, and the lower side the more excit- able. In the first of these types, light acting on the organ from above gives rise to a positive heliotropic response. In the latter, long-continued application of strong light from above gives rise to internal diffusion of stimulus, causing concavity of the more excitable lower half, with the result of negative heliotropic response. Intermediate between these we have seen that there are cases of the equal excitation ot the upper and lower halves of the organ, bringing about the so-called dia-heliotropic position. As examples of the two extreme types—within which lie the responses of all ordinary leaves—I shall here give records of the movements of leaves of Mango (Mangzfera indica) and of Artocarpus. Positive type of response: Mango.—The leaves of this plant when young are bent abruptly downwards by the sharp curvature of the short petiole. In the course of a week or so, however, they rise, and gradually attain a position at or above the horizontal, a process in which the action of light is an important agent. This will be seen from the following record of the movement of a Mango leaf when acted upon by sunlight from above (fig. 263). The record before the application of light was practically horizontal, showing that there was little natural movement; but the DIA-HELIOTROPISM AND DIA-GEOTROPISM 653 application of light induced an energetic movement upwards— that is to say, a positive heliotropic response, the tip of the leaf moving through a distance of 35 mm in the course of sixty-five minutes— that is to say, at an average rate of +53 mm. per minute. On the cessation of light there was a slow recovery, which was found to be only partial. Negative type of response: Artocar- pus.—As an example of negative response by the internal diffusion of stimulus, due to the high transverse con- “19” 20’ 30’ 40” 50’ 60’ 70’ 80’ 90° ductivity of the tissue, —_ IRS ; ; FIG. 263. Positive Heliotropic Movement of and the greater excita- Leaf of MJangifera zndica under Sunlight bility of the lower half acting from Above Se a 5 Dotted line shows recovery on cessation of of the pulvinoid, I give ces here two records, from leaves of different ages, borne on the same plant, under the action of strong sunlight from above (fig. 264). The upper of these two records was taken from a young leaf, which was second in order from the top of the shoot, and the lower from the fourth. It is generally found that motile sensitiveness is at its greatest in leaves which are neither too young nor too old. In the present experiment the upper of the two leaves, which was very young, gave a negative heliotropic response, the tip moving through 15 mm. in the course of eighty minutes, or at an average rate of almost ‘2 mm. per minute. In the case of the lower leaf the rate of the responsive move- ment was more rapid, being on an average ‘5 mm.per minute. As regards leaves, then, their responsive movements under light have been shown to be very definite, and simply 654 PLANT RESPONSE determined by the fact that it is always the more excited side of the motile organ that undergoes contraction and concavity. That various types of this effect arise is due only to the unequal excitabilities of the two halves, and to the fact that the stimulus remains in some cases localised, while in others, owing to the better transverse con- ductivity of the tissue, it becomes internally diffused. The position ultimately taken up by the leaves is thus determined primarily by the action of light, and secondarily by that of various subsidiary factors, cor which are: (1) the natural Fic, 264. Negative Heliotopic Response movement of the organ, acting from Above due to hyponasty or epi- The upper record exhibits the response of nasty ; (2) the differen- avery young leaf, second in order from : , He the top of theshoot. Thelower record _ tial excitability of the shows that of an older leaf, fourth in two halves of the dorsi- order on the same shoot. ventral organ under the stimulus of gravity ; (3) the turgescent condition of the plant, as determined by its suctional activity ; and (4) the limits of flexibility of the organ, as determined by its anatomical peculiarities. It is out of the innumerable possible combina- tions of these factors that the variety of attitudes ultimately assumed by the leaves directly arises. With regard to the nature of geotropic action on leaves, it will be shown in the next chapter that here also, as in the case of dia-heliotropism, the assumption of a dia-geotropic property is unnecessary ; for the observed effects are ex- plained by the fact, which I shall demonstrate, that a dorsi- ventral organ possesses differential geotropic sensibility. DIA-HELIOTROPISM AND DIA-GEOTROPISM 655 SUMMARY The position ultimately assumed by a leaf is determined by heliotropic action and other subsidiary factors. These subsidiary factors are: (1) the natural movement of the organ, due to epinasty or hyponasty ; (2) the differ- ential excitability to gravitational stimulus of the two halves of the organ; (3) the effect of suctional activity and of turgescence; and (4) the modification of effect by the characteristic limits of flexibility of the organ. As regards the action of light, the lamina is not the perceptive organ, and there is no specific dia-heliotropic sensitiveness possessed by the leaves. The sensibility of the pulvinoid of a leaf is essentially similar to that of a pulvinus. There are two main types of response given by pulvinoids: (1) that exhibited when the conductivity of the organ is feeble, and vertical light induces movement upwards, or posztive response ; and (2) that exhibited when conductivity is great, and the lower half is the more excitable, inducing movement downwards, or negative response. The various attitudes assumed by the leaves are the joint effects of these responsive actions, and their modification by epinasty or hyponasty, the differential action of gravity, the turgescent condition of the plant, and the limits of flexibility of the pulvinus or pulvinoid. CHAPTER IXLYE TORSIONAL RESPONSE TO HELIOTROPIC AND GEOTROPIC STIMULUS: AUTONOMOUS TORSION AND ITS VARIATIONS Torsional effect—Method of recording torsional response—Torsional response under the lateral action of light—Torsional response to other forms of lateral stimulation—Torsional response of compound strip of ebonite and stretched india-rubber— Modification of torsional response by artificial variation of the relative excitabilities of the two halves—Laws of torsional response—Demon- stration of differential geotropic excitation in a dorsi ventral organ— Torsional response to lateral geotropic stimulation—Modification of torsional geotropic response by artificial variation of differential excitability—Autonomous torsion : effect of temperature—Effect of light—Effect of electrical current—Effect of gravity—The twining of stems. Ir has been shown in the course of the last chapter that the movement of the leaf in response to light constitutes a simple instance of that general reaction of plant-tissues to stimulus with which we have now become familiar, and that no specific sensibility requires to be postulated in order to account for it. Torsional effect.—There is one effect, however, which has hitherto appeared to be inexplicable, except on the supposition that some such specific sensibility had actually been acquired by the leaf for the definite purpose of sub- serving the advantage of the plant by placing the upper surface of the lamina at right angles to incident light. A leaf when struck laterally by light undergoes a torsion, which carries the upper surface of the lamina into such a position that it faces the light. No result could seem at first sight more conclusively to support the theory of dia-heliotropic sensibility. Before going further into this question, I shall give records of the actual effect observed. In fig. 265 is shown a curve which exhibits the increasing torsion induced HELIOTROPIC TORSIONAL RESPONSE 657 by lateral application of sunlight in the terminal leaflet of Desmodium. A light index was attached transversely to the lamina, by means of which, and with the help of a pro- tractor, the gradually increasing torsion was measured at definite intervals of time, It will be seen from the curve FIG, 265. Torsional Response of Terminal Leaflet of Desmodium under the Action of Lateral Sunlight Abscissa represents time in minutes, and ordinate angular movement in degrees. thus obtained, of which the abscissa represents time, and the ordinate the induced angular torsion, that in this case, within twenty-five minutes, a torsion of 14° had been induced. Method of recording torsional response.—For the purpose of certain investigations, presently to be described, in connection with this torsion, it became necessary to devise special experimental arrangements by which a continuous record of the rate of torsion and its variations could be obtained. The mode in which this was accomplished will be understood from the illustration in fig. 266. A mirror, carried on a light spring-clip made of aluminium, is slipped over and attached to the petiole, at a short distance from the pulvinus or pulvinoid, which is the seat of the torsional movement. As the object is to record only the torsion, the UU 658 PLANT RESPONSE vertical up and down movement, if there be any, is prevented by a lateral support, which has at its end a smooth bent rod of glass, in the concavity of which the free petiole rests. If there be any responsive torsional movement, a spot of light reflected from the mirror will now undergo a vertical deflec- tion. This is, for convenience of the record, converted into lateral by reflection from a second mirror suitably inclined. The response record is then taken in the usual manner on Fic. 266. Torsion-recorder M, mirror slipped over petiole by light aluminium clip behind; G, bent glass piece supporting petiole to prevent vertical movement. a revolving drum. The absolute value of the angular movement shown in the record can be determined from a knowledge of the distance from the mirror of the recording drum. ! Torsional response under the lateral action of light.—The pulvinus of the leaf, say of Mzmosa, is now stimulated by throwing upon it a horizontal beam of light, which strikes it laterally. By the word /ateral is here meant either the right or left flank of the pulvinus or pulvinoid, consisting of part of the upper and part of the lower aniso- tropic halves. This causes a responsive torsion, by which the petiole is rotated, the tendency being for the upper, or less excitable, half to face the stimulating agent. The up curve, then, in this and the following records will represent a torsional movement by which the less excitable upper side is made to HELIOTROPIC TORSIONAL RESPONSE 659 face the source of stimulus. In the present case (fig. 267) we see that the responsive torsion took place during the application of light, and that on the cessation of stimulus there was a positive after-effect, followed by recovery. If light were applied laterally on the opposite flank, the torsion would be found to take place in the opposite direction, the law which determines such movement being, as said before, that the less excitable is always turned towards the stimulus. In connection with this, we have to notice, in the first place, that the responsive torsion takes place not when the lamina, but when the Fic. 267. Torsional Re- pulvinus, is acted on laterally by sponse of Leaf of AZimosa licht : d dl th ficul when Laterally Stimu- ight ; and, secondly, the particular lated by Sunlight torsion in question results from the he ordinate gives the ab- differential action of stimulus, by its solute angular movement : - in degrees. The dotted lateral application to a complex line shows positive after- organ, the lower half of which is the effect and _ recovery on i stoppage of light. more excitable. Torsional response to other forms of lateral stimu- lation.—The supposition that this torsional response is due to a specific sensibility to light, evolved for the advantage of the plant, will be found entirely untenable if it can be shown that the same movement is manifested under the same conditions in response to other forms of stimulus. Thus, on bringing a heated wire near that side which was previously excited by light, I obtained exactly the same torsional response of the same sign. Still more interesting is the excitation of the same lateral side by chemical stimulus, say a strong solution of common salt. In the record given (fig. 268) we see how similar in every way are this response and that evoked by light. The upward arrow (t) indicates the application of chemical stimulus to the same flank as had previously been stimulated by light, of which the record is eu 2 660 PLANT RESPONSE given in fig, 267, The downward arrow (J) indicates the application of the chemical solution to the opposite flank. It will be noticed that we have in consequence, in the dotted portion of the record,a reversal of the first torsional response. Torsional response of compound strip of ebonite and stretched india-rubber.— Enough has already been said to demonstrate the fact that the torsion induced in leaves by the lateral application of light is not due to any specific sensibility to light as such. I shall next therefore proceed to show that the same torsion is the mechanical result of the differential Fic. 268. Torsional Response of Leaf Fic. 269. Torsional Re- of Mimosa to Lateral Chemical sponse of Complex Strip Stimulus of Ebonite and India- rubber under Lateral Action of Strong Radia- tion Upward arrow (%) indicates moment of application of stimulus to one flank ; (|) indicates application of stimulus to opposite flank, with effect of reversing torsional movement. contraction of a complex organ, which is fixed at one end, and subjected to lateral stimulation. I have in a former chapter described an artificial model of the pulvinus of Mimosa, which consisted of a compound strip, the upper half of which was ebonite, and the lower the more contractile stretched india-rubber. If sucha strip be held securely at one end in aclamp, and if the lateral flank, consisting half of ebonite and half of india-rubber, be subjected to the strong action of light, records being taken in the usual manner, it will be found that a torsional response takes place which. is in every respect similar to that of the pulvinus of a HELIOTROPIC TORSIONAL RESPONSE 661 leaf, the less contractile ebonite being turned so as to face the light (fig. 269). Modification of torsional response by artificial varia- tion of the relative excitabilities of the two halves.— I find it necessary to go still further into this subject of torsional response, as by its means I have been enabled to solve a question of very great importance, that, namely, of the different excitabilities of the two halves of the anisotropic organ to geotropic stimulus. The fact that it is the differential character of the excitabilities of these two halves that brings about the torsion of a dorsi-ventral organ under lateral stimu- lation may be still further established in a very interesting manner by inducing artificial variation in the existing excita- bilities. The torsion depends, as said before, on the difference of excitability as between the two. If,then, we could render the lower and more excitable half gradually less and less excitable, till its differential excitability had disappeared, the organ would thus have been rendered virtually radial. The torsional effect might then be expected to vanish, and a simple curvature towards stimulus to result, without torsion, as in the case of other radial organs. Let us next suppose the reduction of excitability to be carried still further, till the lower half of the pulvinus have been rendered less excitable than the upper. On the theory of torsional response which has just been advanced, it is the less excitable lower half which should now twist round to face the stimulating light. In other words, there would then be a reversal of the original torsion. Thus, as the excitability of the lower half becomes gradually reduced, the intensity of the normal positive torsional response by which the upper half was made to face the stimulus would be gradually first decreased to zero, and then reversed to negative, as the excitability of the lower became first equal to, and then less than, that of the upper half. If, on the other hand, the excitability of the wfper half be reduced, the existing differential excitability as between upper and lower would then be still further increased, and the intensity of the positive torsional response would be 662 PLANT RESPONSE enhanced. All these conclusions will be found exactly verified in the records given in fig. 270. In the first part of the left-hand figure we see the normal positive torsional response of M/cmosa under the lateral action of light. The excitability of the lower half of the pulvinus was then reduced by the local appli- cation of chloroform, at the moment represented by the arrow from: below (ft). It will be seen that the response now undergoes _ reversal, owing to the fact that it is the upper side that is the relatively more excit- able. In the figure to the right, again, is shown the effect on the torsional re- sponse of the leaf, of zxcreas- wg the already existing FIG. tion of Torsional Response under 270. Records showing Modifica- Induced Variations of Differential Excitabilities in Pulvinus of AZ¢mosa In left-hand figure the normal torsion is seen to be reversed by application of chloroform to the lower half (f), reversing the differential excitability of the organ, In the right-hand record the normal responsive torsion is enhanced by application of chloro- form to the upper half (|), increasing the natural differential excitability of difference as between the ex- citabilities of the two halves, when that of the upper is reduced by the local applica- the organ. 2 ; ? tion of chloroform. It will be seen that this application as marked by the arrow from above (|) caused an enhancement of torsional response, as seen in the greater steepness of the curve. Laws of torsional response.—From these experiments we arrive at the following laws: 1. An anisotropic organ, when laterally excited, under- goes torsion, by which the less excitable side is made to face the stimulus. 2. Stimulus remaining constant, the intensity of torsional response increases with the differential excitability. But when the original difference is in any way reduced or reversed, the torsional response undergoes concomitant diminution or reversal, GEOTROPIC TORSIONAL RESPONSE 663 3. An organ which, under lateral stimulation, responds by torsion, is always physiologically anisotropic, and the side which is made to face the stimulus is the less excitable. Demonstration of differential geotropic excitation in a dorsi-ventral organ.—From this experimental demon- stration, then, we have obtained a new means of discrimi-~ nating the relative excitabilities of the two halves of an organ, since that side which is turned by the responsive movement to face the given stimulus is relatively the less excitable to it. It will be remembered that we found that the reason why certain dorsi-ventral organs showed a tendency to assume a horizontal position under the action of vertical light was not that they possessed dia-heliotropic sensibility, but was due to the differential excitability of the two halves under stimulus in general, including that of light. Again, in the case of the action of gravity, it is found that such organs exhibit a similar tendency to place themselves horizontally ; and the assumption of a specific dia-geotropic sensibility is not necessary, if it can be shown that the upper and lower sides are unequally sensitive to this stimulus also. We may first take an ideally simple case. We have seen that in a radial apogeotropic stem, when laid hori- zontally, it is only the upper half that is effectively stimulated by geotropic action; and there was reason to believe that this was due to the fact that it was the inner tangential wall of the upper side—in contrast to the less excitable outer wall of the lower side—that was excited by the statolithic or other influence of weight (pp. 495, 503). If now, for any reason, the excitability of the upper half of the horizontally placed radial organ be abolished, the geotropic response of that effective half will disappear, and the organ will remain horizontal, as if unaffected by stimulus of gra- vity. This state of things we have already realised, when the excitability of the upper half was artificially diminished by the local application of cold, and geotropic response was seen to be arrested (p. 504). Now, a horizontally placed radial organ which has been rendered unequally excitable 664 PLANT RESPONSE in the two halves, by the reduction of excitability of the upper, is virtually a plagiotropic or dorsi-ventral organ, and we can thus see why a true plagiotropic organ, laid hori- zontally, has a tendency to remain in that position. It would remain in that position absolutely if the excitability of the outer tangential wall of the lower side were actually ~ zero, and the general excitability of the upper half of the dorsi-ventral organ had also completely vanished; but if these two values were not both zero, various effects would occur, according to the relative differential excitabilities of the two halves. I shall now describe the line of investigation by which it is possible to demonstrate the fact that the two halves of an anisotropic organ possess different excitabilities with regard to geotropic stimulus ; but before entering upon this, it is necessary to obtain a clear idea of the direction of response, in relation to direction of the force which causes stimulation of gravity. In the case of an apogeotropic stem, laid horizontally, we have vertical lines of force striking the organ from above, and the curvature induced makes it turn upwards to meet, as it were, these lines of force. We have here an example which is analogous to the response of the organ to the rays of light, in which the responsive move- ment consists in a similar curvature upwards to meet them. The fact that the upper and lower halves of an aniso- tropic organ are unequally excited by light has just been demonstrated by means of torsional response, where it was shown that lateral excitation induced a twisting movement by which ¢he less excitable was made to face the incident rays. By a similar torsional response of the anisotropic organ we are able not only to demonstrate the unequal geotropic excitability of the upper and lower halves, but also, by noting which of the two is made to face the lines of gravita- tional force, to determine which it is that is the less excitable to this particular form of stimulus. Torsional response to lateral geotropic stimulation.— For this experiment I took a leaf of Exythrina indica, whose — =. GEOTROPIC TORSIONAL RESPONSE 665 pulvinus I find to be very sensitive to geotropic action. Under normal conditions the leaflets of this plant place themselves horizontally. I now took one of these, say the terminal, and adjusted the plant so that its lateral edge was vertical. The dorsal and ventral halves of its pulvinus were thus equally subjected to geotropic action. This experiment, it is to be noted, was carried out in a dark room, where the only stimulus acting was geotropic. It will be seen, from the first part of the left-hand record in fig. 271, that under geotropic stimulus a torsional response was here obtained, which was exactly similar to that given under the action of light in fig. 270. The angular movement was in this case at the rate of about ‘11° per minute, and it was the dorsal surface of the organ that was eventually turned upwards— that is to say, so as to face the lines of force of gravity. This experiment, then, conclusively demonstrates the fact that the two halves of the dorsi-ventral organ are unequally sensitive to geotropic stimulus, and that it is the upper which is the less sensitive. Modification of torsional geotropic response by arti- ficial variation of differential excitability.—The fact that the upper half is, under normal conditions, the less excitable to geotropic stimulus is capable of further and striking demonstration, by the tests of reversal, and acceleration of torsional response. I have already explained, as will be remembered, that if the existing difference of excitability were reversed, the direction of torsional response would also be reversed ; and that if, on the contrary, the difference were by any means increased, the rate of torsional movement would be correspondingly accelerated. __In the first part of fig. 271 is seen the natural torsional response of the terminal leaflet of Erythrina indica. The excitability of the lower half of the pulvinus was now abolished, by the local applica- tion of chloroform, at the point marked with an upward arrow, and the torsional response to geotropic stimulation was thus found to be reversed, from plus 11° to minus °O5° per minute. Hence it was now the lower half of the pul- 666 PLANT RESPONSE vinus—artificially rendered the less excitable of the two— that was found turning upwards, to face the rays of incident force of gravity. The converse of this experiment would consist in still further reducing the excitability of the upper half of the pulvinus, in which case the normal differential excitability of the two halves would be increased, and the geotropic response enhanced. The right-hand record in fig. 271 shows that this is what actually occurs. The first part of the record displays the normal torsional response, and that which follows, after the local application of chloroform on _ the upper surface-——indicated by the arrow from above Fic. 271. Records showing Torsional —exhibits, by its in- Response to Geotropic Stimulus and ; se : Induced Modifications in Terminal Leaf- creased steepness, the en- let of Erythrina indica hanced character of this In the figure to the left is seen the normal response. torsion under lateral geotropic action, it and its reversal when the differential We have thus seen, excitability of the organ is reversed by by means of teratenelaees the application of chloroform to the ; lower half (t). In the figure to the sponse, that the differ- right is seen the enhancement of the F torsional response when the natural ential character of the differential excitability is increased by excitability of the two the application of chloroform to the ’ ; upper half of the organ (|). halves of an anisotropic organ under the stimulus of light is in every way paralleled by their differential ex- citability to the stimulus of gravity. The geotropic response of the organ, as a whole, is neither simple positive nor negative, but may be more fittingly described as differential. And as we saw with regard to photic stimulus that there was no need for the assumption of any specific dia-heliotropic sensibility in plagiotropic stems or in leaves—their move- ments being fully accounted for by the mechanical con- siderations arising out of their differential excitability to AUTONOMOUS TORSION 667 stimulus in general—so similarly with regard to geotropic stimulus, there is no necessity for the assumption of any specific dia-geotropic sensibility. Autonomous torsion.—We have now seen how torsional response is induced in an anisotropic organ. We have next to deal with that interesting class of phenomena which consists of the natural torsional movements of growing organs. It is to be remembered that, in referring to these torsional movements, we mean the torsional growth-move- ments of certain stems themselves about their own axes. The term fosztzve torsion in such cases means a movement which appears, when looked at from above, to take place in the same direction as the movement of the hands of a watch, the term zegative being used in the contrary sense. Such torsional movements are found to occur in the stems of climbing plants. In some of these they are of a positive, and in others of a negative character, while in still others again they alternate, the natural torsion being, say, positive at one period, and negative at another. Now, we have seen in the case of leaves that, owing to different rates of growth in the antagonistic upper and lower halves of the petiole, autonomous epinastic or hyponastic movements occur in a rectilinear direction. When the growth of the upper half is predominant, the movement of the organ is downwards, and wice versa. More complex, however, is that case in which the line of maximum growth is a spiral revolving about an axis, thus bringing about a growth-movement which causes a torsion of the organ round that axis. In the case of epinasty and hyponasty, it was said that the rectilinear movement induced was due, not toa total absence of growth on either side, but to the relatively greater growth of one of the antagonistic halves. Similarly, in these torsional movements we have to remember the existence of antagonistic elements in the stem, and it is the predominant growth of one of these, the right-handed or left- handed, that brings about the resultant positive or negative torsion. 668 PLANT RESPONSE Effect of temperature on autonomous torsion.—In order to take a record of these torsional movements, a stem is taken and held securely fixed at one end—say the lower— the other being left free. On this free end, the mirror, by the reflected light from which the record is to be made, is attached ; and a long thread from above is tied to the tip of the specimen, in order to prevent its falling to one side by its own weight. The limpness of this supporting thread allows the torsional movements of the specimen to occur without hindrance. In taking records of the natural torsional response of plants at various temperatures, I have obtained results which are in a general way similar to the records made of longi- tudinal growth-response (p. 446)—that is to say, the response is enhanced up to the optimum, which is at or near 35°C. Above this, response is diminished. These facts will be seen from the following table, which gives the absolute rate of the angular movements of the torsional response at different temperatures. The specimens employed in these and the following experiments were climbing stems of Porana paniculata, a plant belonging to the Campanulacee, which normally ex- hibits a strong negative torsion. TABLE GIVING THE ABSOLUTE RATE OF ANGULAR MOVEMENTS or TORSIONAL RESPONSE AT VARIOUS TEMPERATURES Temperature } Torsional movements . | 2 a 28°C ‘09° per minute g20.C, “18° oe 2216s Depry | 37° C. 80h Ris | An interesting effect was observed, however, in this case of torsional response, when the temperature was raised above 43°C., which was apparently different from what occurred in ordinary longitudinal growth-response at the same temperature. It was found in the latter case that at 44° C. or thereabouts growth underwent an apparent arrest ; AUTONOMOUS TORSION 669 but this was shown not to be due to any actual arrest brought about by heat-rigor, for the rhythmic activity induced at this high temperature was even greater than before (p. 432). Now,in the case of torsional response, I find that though the rate of torsion undergoes a continuous decrease up to 42°C. or 43°C., yet beyond this an unex- pected increase is induced. Thus in the experiment just described, the torsional movement at 45° C. was at least temporarily enhanced to ‘8° per minute. This phenomenon may be due to abnormal relaxation at these high temperatures. Or there is another possible explanation. It has been said that the resultant torsion is due to the differential growth- activities of two antagonistic elements. Now, it may be that at 44° C. or thereabouts the growth of the less excitable of these antagonistic elements may undergo the same kind of arrest as we have seen to occur in the longitudinal growth of a radial organ. The sudden increase observed at and above 44° C, in the rate of natural torsion may then be due to the withdrawal of the resistance previously offered by the growth- activity of the antagonistic element, Effect of increased suctional activity.—Another in- teresting point, in connection with the occurrence of auto- nomous torsion, lies in the fact that its rate is enhanced by any means which tends to increase internal energy. One example of this has just been seen in the effect induced by rise of temperature. Another is found in the application of warm water to the base of the specimen, a process which has already been shown, in the case of radial organs, to cause, by means of the consequent increase of suctional activity, a sudden enhancement of the rate of autonomous growth (p. 430). I find, similarly, that the application of warm water to its base enhances the torsional response of a torsion- ing plant. Effect on natural torsion of unilateral application of light.—We have seen that when an anisotropic organ is laterally excited by external stimulus, a response torsion takes place, by which the less excitable side is made to face 670 PLANT RESPONSE the stimulus. Now, in a naturally torsioning organ, such an induced torsional movement must obviously be opposite in direction to the natural movement caused by internal energy. This will be found to be illustrated in the modification in- duced in autonomous torsion by the unilateral application of light, as shown in fig. 272, where the first part of the curve shows the normal negative torsion of the plant. At the point marked with the upward arrow, light from a thirty-two candle-power electric lamp was allowed to strike the stem from one side. It will be seen that the excitatory effect of this external stimulus is first exhibited in the retardation of Fic. 272. Retardation and Reversal of Normal Torsional Movement by the unilateral Action of Light in Porana paniculata Light applied at + retards, and in the course of two minutes reverses, the normal movement of negative torsion, making it positive. On cessation of light at on . there is recovery of the original negative torsion, the normal rate of torsion, culminating in its abolition, suc- ceeded by actual reversal of direction. The normal negative was thus converted into positive torsion. On the cessation of the external stimulus the normal torsion was gradually resumed. The effect described is best observed in vigorous specimens in which the natural torsional movement is fairly strong. In studying the action of light on autonomous longi- tudinal growth, we found that when a series of responses to the action of light were recorded, the first effect of incident AUTONOMOUS TORSION 671 light, if the specimen were in a sub-tonic condition, would be, by increasing the internal energy, to give rise to an enhanced rate of growth; but when the normal tonic condition had been. attained by absorption of light, the subsequent responses would be by a movement opposite to that of growth-elongation—that is to say, a contraction or retardation of growth. In experimenting on the effect ot light on the autonomous torsional movements of the stems of certain specimens of /pomaa, I have met with results exactly parallel—that is to say, the natural rate of torsional movement, in this case negative, was transitorily enhanced by the first incidence of light, but declined and underwent reversal after continued action of stimulus. In the second and subsequent responses this preliminary enhancement was found to have disappeared, the effect of light now being a retardation and subsequent reversal of the natural torsional movement. Effect of electric current.—I have often observed a very interesting effect, as induced by a constant current flowing along the length of the organ. During the continu- ance of the current -the normal torsional response is first decreased and then reversed. On the cessation of current there is recovery and restoration of the normal direction of torsion. If the current maintained be strong or long con- tinued, the induced reversal may become more or less persistent. Such an induced reversal of torsion, moreover, is independent of the direction of current. Effect of gravity.—I shall next describe a series of effects which I have only been able to obtain with any degree of certainty under favourable conditions. Where natural torsion is feeble the retarding effect of gravity which I am about to describe is such as to arrest torsion, and it is difficult to decide whether such arrest is accidental or in- duced. When the specimen, on the other hand, is too vigorous, the retardation is not easily observed, probably because it tends to be masked by the natural movement. In practice, therefore, the best specimens are those characterised 672 PLANT RESPONSE by moderate vigour and uniformity of natural torsion during a considerable length of time. In studying the effect of geotropic stimulus on radial organs, we have seen that its effective intensity was greater when the specimen was inclined at an angle of 135° to the vertical than when at 90° (p. 501). It occurred to me, then, that the effect of geotropic stimulus might possibly be detected, by means of variations induced in natural torsional response at different angles of inclination. Thus, we might first take a record of the normal rate of torsion, occurring when the tip of the organ was in its natural position upwards. We might next take a record of torsion, the stem being held horizontal ; and we might finally take a record with the tip held vertically downwards.'! The effect of geotropic stimulus might then be seen in the retardation induced in the normal torsion. I give here a summary of results obtained from two different specimens of Porana paniculata, and from a specimen of [pomea. In the first case, the record taken in the normal upright position gave a rate of movement of negative torsion through minus twenty-three divisions of the scale per minute. The record of torsion in the horizontal position was found to give a very much diminished rate, being now only mzzzus seven divi- sions per minute ; and finally, with the tip held downwards, the torsional movement of the specimen was found to have undergone an absolute reversal to positive—that is to say, the tip now moved wth the hands of a watch at a rate of plus five divisions of the scale per minute. In the second case, the normal negative torsion in the erect position was at the rate of mus twenty divisions per 1 Tere we must bear in mind, with regard to the effective angle of inclination, a certain difference as between radial and torsioning organs. In the former, the line of growth may be taken as vertical, and coincident with the axis, whereas in the latter it is spiral, or inclined at some undetermined angle to the axis of the organ. Hence any inclination of the growth-line of a radial organ is measured by the angle between the organ and the vertical. In the case of the torsioning organ, however, when it is held vertically downwards, its growth-line makes an angle—not of 180° to the vertical, but—of 180° less by the degree of its inclination to the axis, ANTONOMOUS TORSION 673 minute. This was reduced in the horizontal position to minus fourteen divisions per minute, and in the vertically downward position it was found to have undergone reversal to plus nine divisions per minute. In the next series of experiments I took records from specimens held alternately up and down and up again. This was dove in order to eliminate the effect of any chance variation. The specimen employed was /fomwa. The tor- sional response in the first up position was at the rate of minus sixteen divisions per minute. When held in the in- verted position, the rate was found reduced to mznus ten divisions per minute, and when once more placed in its normal vertical position, the /fomwa exhibited an increased rate of movement—that is to say, the torsion now took place at the rate of szzzas twenty divisions of the scale per minute. All these results tend to show that the action of stimulus of gravity is to retard the autonomous torsion, this effect being at a maximum when the specimen is in a position at 180° from the vertically upright. The twining of stems.—The twining movements of certain stems are the result of various contributing factors, the relative values of which may differ in different cases. One such factor, suggested by Von Mohl and denied by others, may lie in the irritability of the stem to the contact of its support. Such response to unilateral pressure was found to occur in various organs (p. 497), and probably plays an important part, in some cases, in the phenomenon of twining. Some connection would also seem to exist, in many in- stances, between autonomous torsion and twining. In the first place, most of the twining plants also exhibit autonomous torsion. Again, just as we have various types of torsioning organs—some characterised by positive, others negative, and others again by positive alternating with negative, or negative with positive, torsions—so in twining stems also, we see some which exhibit positive-directioned twining, others negative, and others again alternately one and the other. A plant, moreover, which has twined, shows, if inverted, a tendency x xX 674 PLANT RESPONSE towards reversal or untwining. This is analogous to the effect of inversion on autonomous torsion, in which, as we have seen, the original torsion tends to be reversed. The effect of gravity is known to be a very important factor in the induction of twining. It is believed that we have here a third type of geotropic action, which is neither positive nor negative, but lateral in its character. Under lateral geotropism, a curvature is induced in the twining stem in a horizontal plane. Since we have found, however, that even such diverse effects as positive and negative geotropic actions are not to be regarded as due to different specific sensibilities, it would be interesting to inquire whether lateral geotropism also is not simply a case of ordinary apogeo- tropism in combination with some other tendency, say that of autonomous torsion. At any rate, that this erectile or apogeotropic action is always an element in the process, is shown by the fact that the coils in the older portion of the stem become drawn out, show- ing that the ascending movement predominates in that region. The whole subject is, however, extremely complicated, owing to the presence of many factors and their different relative intensities. And each of these may again be subject to modification, under the action of external stimulus, as was seen to be the case with autonomous torsion under the in- fluence of light, gravity, and the variation of internal energy. SUMMARY The observed effect of torsion, by which the upper surface of the leaf is turned to face the light, is not due to any specific dia-heliotropic sensibility. Similar torsional response is induced by the lateral application of any other form of stimulus, such as thermal or chemical. Such torsional response is also obtained from a com- pound strip made of two unequally contracting inorganic materials, such as ebonite and stretched india-rubber, when stimulus is applied to one of the lateral flanks. TORSIONAL RESPONSE TO STIMULUS 675 The general law of induced torsional response is that the less excitable side of the organ is made to face the incident stimulus. If the excitability of the lower half of the pulvinus be artificially abolished by the local application of chloro- form, the torsional responsive movement is reversed. The leaf now executes a movement by which its lower surface is made to face the light. On artificially increasing the natural difference, as between the two halves, by altogether abolishing the excitability of the upper, the normal torsional response is accelerated. That the upper and lower halves of a dorsi-ventral organ are unequally sensitive to geotropic stimulus is shown by the fact that they give torsional response, under which the less excitable half is carried so as to face the incident lines of gravitational force. An artificial increase of this natural difference of excitability has the effect of accelerating the rate of torsional response. The artificial reversal, on the other hand, of these relative excitabilities has the effect of reversing the direction of the natural responsive move- ment. From these considerations it is seen that none of the responsive movements caused in dorsi-ventral organs by light or gravitation are due to any specific dia-heliotropic or dia-geotropic sensibility, and that they are in reality due to the differential excitability of the two halves of the organ. The autonomous torsional movement of growth increases in rate up to an optimum temperature, after which it begins to decrease. There may be a second acceleration after the attainment of the first minimum. This autonomous torsional movement is retarded or even reversed in sign by unilateral stimulus of light. A constant current, flowing along the length of the organ, often retards or reverses the normal autonomous torsion. The stimulus of gravity also has the effect of retarding or reversing normal torsional movement. The effective intensity of this stimulus appears to be greatest when the organ acted upon is in a position at 180° from the vertically upright. %% 2 CHAPTER XLVIII NYCTITROPIC MOVEMENTS Comparison between nyctitropic and autonomous pulsations—Diurnal movement of plagiotropic stem—Supposed distinction between nyctitropic and other movements of response to stimulus of light—Diurnal response of ‘leaf of Biophytum—Diurnal response of primary petiole of J/:mosa—Periodic impulses acting on the leaf—Periodic impulses contributed by the plant as a whole— Other modes of exhibition of diurnal periodicity of hydrostatic tension —Forced vibration and its periodic after-effects— Physical analogue—Impressed periodic vibrations in organ originally radial. IN addition to the other effects of light which we have been studying, there is also a periodic movement, induced by the alternation of day and night, which consists of a pulsation executed by the responding organs in the very long period of twenty-four hours. In its pulsatory character this move- ment resembles the so-called autonomous pulsations of such leaflets as those of Desmodium gyrans. But besides the fact that it has the long and definite duration of twenty-four hours, whereas the autonomous pulsations of the leaflets of Desmodium are short and variable in period, there are other differences between the two. Comparison between nyctitropic and autonomous pulsations.—In the first place it will be noticed with regard to the daily periodic movement that at any given time all the motile organs of the same plant are going through the same phase. This rhythm therefore is in a manner controlled by the plant as a whole. In the case of autonomous movements so called, on the other hand, the seat of rhythm is localised in the motile organ of that particular leaflet whose pulsations are being observed, and there is no necessary coincidence of period, as between any two leaflets on the same plant, The NYCTITROPIC MOVEMENTS 677 period of autonomous vibration is much modified, again, by rise or fall of temperature, and other conditions ; whereas the daily period is unaffected by these circumstances. This is due to the fact that the autonomous pulsations are akin to free or natural vibrations, whereas the daily periodic move- ments are, as we shall see, of the nature of forced vibra- tions. As the nyctitropic movement in the primary petiole of Mimosa is of a simple character, free from those other complicating considerations which have to be taken into account in the case of the leaflets, this organ has been chosen as the typical specimen by many investigators. We shall therefore consider in detail all its peculiarities in regard to this movement, but I shall at the same time endeavour by the use of the comparative method to trace the evolution of the movement, as first seen in plagiotropic stems, more clearly manifested in certain dorsi-ventral organs like ordinary leaves, and strongly exhibited in pulvinated leaves, such as those of Mimosa. Supposed distinction between nyctitropic and other movements of response to stimulus of light.—This nycti- tropic movement has been sharply distinguished from ordinary response to the stimulation of light, by the statement that while the latter depends on the direction, this is determined only by the varying zntenszty of illumination. As a further distinction, it is also insisted that, whereas the responsive curvature induced by light takes place in all directions, nyctitropic movement always occurs in a single definite plane. But whenever we subject the pulvinus of J/zmosa to the action of strong light, we obtain response by a greater or smaller depression or fall, which occurs in a definite vertical plane, and is precisely the same in character as that which is exhibited slowly during the course of the whole day, and attains its maximum in the evening. This response to strong light, moreover, is always the same, on whichever side of the pulvinus the stimulus may act—that is to say, it is independent of the direction of light. In analysing this case 678 PLANT RESPONSE further (p. 635) we saw that it constituted a true instance of phototropic reaction, and that this particular movement of fall in a definite direction was due to the stimulus being diffused, whether internally or externally, and by differential action inducing concavity of the lower and more excitable half of the pulvinus. In the assumption by the leaf at evening, then, of its lowest position, we have nothing which is specifically different from this responsive movement, as caused by the action of light on the anisotropic organ; but this same fall, when it attains its maximum in the evening, as a phase of the nyctitropic movement, is usually said to be due, not to the stimulus of light, but to the varzatzon in that stimulus, or to the effect of on-coming darkness. Now, if it had been true that the diminution of light, or on-coming of darkness in the evening, had acted in some unknown manner as a stimulus to bring about the fall, then we should have found that such a fall was at that time extremely rapid, and that it persisted on the withdrawal of light during the night. As a matter of fact, however, it is found that this movement of the petiole downwards has been taking place progressively throughout the day, and that the fall of the leaf at evening is merely a continuation of this previous movement, and not markedly more rapid at that time than before, if the increased mechanical moment due to the closing of the secondary petioles and their leaflets be eliminated by their removal. Nor does the petiole remain in this depressed position, but begins after an interval to erect itself, till in the morning, or earlier, it has attained its highest degree of erection. So far, again, from darkness being efficient to cause the depression of the leaf, it is well known that on artificially darkening the Mimosa plant the leaves respond by erection. It is thus clear that we must seek some other explanation of the nyctitropic movement, and as it is known that such periodic movement persists for a time, even when the plant is kept for days in continuous darkness, the inquiry resolves itself into the two questions: (1) how are these diurnal 4 . NYCTITROPIC MOVEMENTS 679 motile impulses generated? and (2) how is it that the move- ment will still persist in the absence of a periodically exciting cause ? Diurnal movement of plagiotropic stem.—-In order to understand clearly the periodic action of light, in induc- ing diurnal movements, it is best to take as our starting- point the simplest type of anisotropic organ, in which this effect may be observed, and for this we may select the plagiotropic stem of, say, Cucurbzta. This creeping stem is acted on, under natural conditions, by vertical light from above, the excitatory effect of which, by long-continued action, reaches the lower and more excitable side. The stimulus thus becomes internally diffused, and there is also a certain amount of externally diffused light from the environ- ment. Henceit happens that, by the contraction of the more excitable lower half, the free end of the stem becomes progres- sively depressed, under the continued action of light, during the course of the day (p. 627). We have seen that in conse- quence of this the greatest depression occurs about the time of evening, and that on the cessation of the stimulus of light there is a recovery, the stem erecting itself more and more as the night advances (fig. 252). Thus this nyctitropic depression at nightfall ts not due to the on-coming of darkness, but repre- sents the cumulative responsive effect o7 the day's tllumina- tion. In fact we may regard the responsive movement down, and recovery up, which are executed in the course of the diurnal period, as parallel to that phenomenon with which we are already familiar, lof a single response to a single transient stimulus. The only difference lies in the fact that, while in this latter case the whole process is completed in a few minutes, in the former the stimulus acts continuously during something like twelve hours, and the recovery is allowed to take place during the course of the night. Thus, in the ordinary case of records of uniform responses to uniform stimuli, the successive stimuli, each lasting for a few seconds, are given at intervals of some minutes, but in the case of the diurnal responses we have a series of stimuli, 680 PLANT RESPONSE each lasting for twelve hours, and the beginning of each separated from the next by a period of twenty-four hours. Diurnal responses of leaf of Biophytum.—From this simple instance of anisotropy we pass on toa more highly differentiated case of dorsi-ventrality, as seen in leaves. We have already seen that the petiole of Azophytum is only provided with a moderately developed pulvinus, and that the petiole also, as a whole, acts as a diffuse pulvinoid. In this case also, by the in- ternally and externally diffused action of light during the course of the day, the petiole is progressively depressed, from its position of highest erection in the morning to its lowest depression in the even- ing. Recovery takes place again in the ab- sence of the stimulus of FIG. 273. Photographic Record of Diurnal light, and the leaf once Movement of Petiole of Azophytum from more assumes its highest 5 P.M. till 9 A.M. i | ! 6PM 9 2PM SAM 6 3AM : position by morning. If, First part of record shows maximum depres- - sion to be reached at 6 P.M. and maximum however, in the course erection at 7 A.M. After the latter hour of the afternoon, say, at the leaf again begins to exhibit its usual : daytime depression. five o clock, the plant be taken to a dark room, then, owing to the positive after-effect, the leaf will still con- tinue to fall for about an hour, and then begin to erect itself. Owing to the daily periodicity impressed on the plant, of which we shall presently speak in greater detail, the leaves continue to exhibit the diurnal movement even when kept in continuous darkness. I give here (fig. 273) an interesting photographic record of the diurnal movement of the leaf of Bzophytum from §5 P.M., when it was brought into the dark room, till 9 A.M. next morning. It will here be seen that, owing to the NYCTITROPIC MOVEMENTS 681 positive after-effect, the leaf continued to fall till 6 P.M., and that after this it erected itself by a series of five pulsations, until its highest position was attained at about 7 A.M. It next began to exhibit the impressed effect of day, and the leaf then fell rapidly. Diurnal response of primary petiole of Mimosa.—In Mimosa we merely find the repetition of those movements whose evolution we have already traced through the plagio- tropic stem and the dorsi-ventral petiole. This is made very Fic. 274. Continuous, Records of the Diurnal Movement during Thirty- six Hours in Two Specimens of J/zmosa The continuous line represents the movement of the one-year old, and the dotted line that of the six-months old specimen. The maximum depression is seen to take place at six in the evening, and the recovery nearly completed by midnight. This maximum erection is slightly augmented at dawn. apparent in the continuous records which I took of the diurnal movement in two specimens of JZzmosa, the records being continued during a period of thirty-six hours (fig. 274). The two plants were placed in an open verandah, and long light indices made, as already described, of peacock’s quills, were so attached to the petiole as to form prolonga- tions. Records were taken on a vertical revolving drum. A vertical thread was suspended in front of the drum, and the point at which the moving index cut this vertical line was marked at every fifteen minutes. Thus the record gives 682 PLANT RESPONSE a measure of the angular movement at different parts of the day. The dotted line shows the diurnal movement of the younger of the two plants, which was six months old, and the continuous line that of the other, which was one year old. The two, as will be seen, are practically the same. It should be mentioned here that, when this record was taken, 6 A.M. and 6 P.M. were the hours of sunrise and sunset, there being no twilight. The records show that the leaves exhibited the erectile or recovery effect with great rapidity during the first part of the night, this movement being almost completed by 1 A.M. After this there was but slight upward movement, until about 7 A.M., from which time onwards, during twelve hours, they fell continuously. The maximum depression was reached at almost exactly 6 P.M., and then the leaves again rose, repeating their former movement of recovery. Periodic impulses acting on the leaf.—The periodic movements down during the day, and up during the night, are thus seen to be due to periodic impulses of stimulus and recovery acting on the responding leaves. It must be pointed out here that the process of recovery is not alto- gether passive. We saw in the records of Mzmosa under light (fig. 256) that, on the cessation of light, the responding leaf continued to move down for a while owing to the positive after-effect. Later, however, owing to the latent energy which it had acquired by the absorption of light, it exhibited the negative after-effect in an erection which carried it beyond the original position. Hence we can see that the increased internal energy due to previous absorption of light plays an important part in that movement of erection which is initiated shortly after nightfall. We may therefore say that the diurnal movements of the leaves are brought about by two alternate periodic impulses, those, namely, of external stimulus and internal energy. The persist- ence of the effect of each of these impulses will depend on the intensity of the two factors respectively, and also on the capacity of the tissue for absorbing stimulus. For example, the positive after-effect by which the leaf continues NYCTITROPIC MOVEMENTS 683 to be depressed, even on the cessation of the stimulating day- light, may, in some cases, be short-lived, and in others may continue for a considerable period. The return movement will in the latter case be somewhat delayed. Again, the internal energy which hastens recovery may, in certain cases, bring about the utmost erection of which the leaf is capable at some time earlier than the ensuing morning. Thus, for example, in the response of Azophytum, the highest position (fig. 273) was attained at about 7 A.M., while in the records of these particular W/zmosa leaves the corresponding point was reached very much earlier, that is to say at I P.M. Periodic impulses contributed by the plant as a whole.—In addition, however, to those periodic impulses whose seat is in the responding leaf, there is another concor- dant impulse contributed by the plant as a whole which intensifies the diurnal movement. The plant is subjected during the day to the stimulus of light, which causes con- traction of the exterior tissues, and thereby tends to drive the water inwards. The turgidity of the cortex is thus progressively diminished during daylight. But at night the water which has thus been driven into the central reservoir of the plant will flow outwards. In this also the absorbed energy will play an important part. The rhythmic activity of the cortical tissues being thereby increased, they will suck water outwards from the central reservoir, just as the rootlets suck it from the ground. The result of this alternation of external stimulus during the day, and internal stimulus during the night, will be a periodic inflow and outflow—a diminu- tion and increase of tension—the first half of the cycle being completed in the daytime, and the second half in the night. I have shown: (p. 46) that the leaf of Mimosa is erected whenever the internal hydrostatic tension is arti- ficially increased, and depressed when it is diminished ; and we have seen how such variations are induced by the alter- nation of day and night, not only in individual petioles, but throughout the plant as a whole. The alternate ebb and flow of the water, from the central reservoir, will thus be 684 PLANT RESPONSE indicated by the periodic erection and depression of all the leaves synchronously, which will thus act, so to speak, as signal-flags. Other modes of exhibition of diurnal periodicity of hydrostatic tension.—There are also other modes besides that of mechanical response of the leaves by which this diurnal periodicity of tension can be detected ; and Millardet has shown that the maximum tension in J7Zzmosa occurs at dawn, when the primary petiole is in its most erect position. The minimum tension, again, occurs in the evening, when the leaf occupies its most depressed position. Kraus, further, has found the organs of the plant diminish in bulk from morning till afternoon, and that the reverse process takes place from afternoon till morning. Growth itself, again, is well known to exhibit a diurnal periodicity. It is interesting, however, to realise that this is simply the mode by which a radial organ exhibits, in a form of longitudinal response, what was otherwise exhibited in J/zmosa as lateral response. The periodic variation of tension induced by the diurnal period has been seen to manifest itself in J/zmosa in two alternate movements, positive and negative. Similarly we shall find, in growth-response, positive and negative variations above and below the normal or average rate. If we take a balanced record of growth, representing its average rate, a downward line will indicate retardation or negative response, while an upward line will indicate an acceleration of growth, or positive response ; and in thus recording variations of the rate of growth, by the balanced method, for a period of twenty-four hours, we obtain, as we have seen (fig. 191), a curve which closely resembles that of the nyctitropic movement. Forced vibration and its periodic after-effects.—We have thus traced out the two alternating impulses—the direct effect of external stimulus and its direct after-effect, and the internal stimulus with the negative after-effect—which induce the forced vibration of the responding organ. ‘The periodic effects of protoplasmic contraction and expansion, induced NYCTITROPIC MOVEMENTS 685 alternately in the plant under alternating light and dark- ness, thus leave a molecular impress, and this impression is deepened by repetition, finding subsequent expression even when the primary alternating cause is absent. The length of time during which such after-oscillation persists will depend, amongst other things, on the depth of the molecular impression. Physical analogue.—An analogous physical phenome- non will make the point clearer. If a wire be taken and subjected to alternating molecular strains, say by giving it alternate positive and negative twists, the wire being held in each of these twisted positions for a time, then, even after stoppage of such alternate twisting movements, the released wire will continue to vibrate to and fro in expression of the release of impressed latent strains, consequent on previous forced vibrations. That such forced vibrations may persist in a plant has been shown by F. Darwin and D. Pertz, who subjected a plant to alternating geotropic stimuli, by which a rhythmic movement was found to persist for a time, even on the stoppage of stimulation. Czapek and Wiesner obtained similar after-vibrations with alternating phototropic stimuli. Impressed periodic vibrations in organ originally radial.—I shall here give a very remarkable instance of such forced periodic vibrations, as induced in an originally radial organ. I had a row of sunflowers planted, in a line which ran accurately east and west, at the season when the sun moved daily in an almost vertical plane. The plants were thus stimulated in the morning from the east, and in the afternoon from the west. All these plants, then, by helio- tropic action, followed the path of the sun during the course of the day, and during the course of the night, again, there was recovery. Att first the diurnal swing east and west was only through a small angle, but under the action of these repeated periodic impulses it became larger and larger, like that of a pendulum under regular and repeated blows. When the plants had grown to a height of one metre, it was 686 PLANT RESPONSE a remarkable sight in the early mornings to see all the six, with their upper halves bent over equally to the east, and in the evenings equally to the west. One curious phenomenon connected with this consisted in the fact that not only was the nightly recovery completed by 1 A.M., but the upper part of the shoot was already carried over to the east, just as we found the J/zmosa leaf to be erected to the highest position by midnight in consequence of the presence of internal energy. Analysis of constituent impulses causing nyctitropic movement.—Nyctitropic movements are thus brought about by two different periodic factors, themselves induced by the periodic action of light and darkness. These periodic forces acting on the pulvinus are: 1. The differential heliotropic effect on the pulvinus itself. By the stimulus of light, externally and internally diffused, the dorsi-ventral leaf is progressively depressed during the day. The reverse process takes place at night, by means of natural recovery, aided by internal energy, which gives an impulse opposite to that of external stimulus. 2. A periodic inflow and outflow of water taking place in the plant as a whole, by the recurrent action of light and darkness. This, acting on the dorsi-ventral pulvinus, causes periodic movement of the petiole. 3. Both these periodic forces are concordant in their action on the pulvinated organ, and give rise to periodic movements of large amplitude. SUMMARY The nyctitropic movement of such leaves as that of Mimosa is believed to be distinguished from _heliotropic action proper by the facts that (1) it takes place in a definite plane, and (2) it is caused, not by light as a constant force, but by its variation of intensity, the fall of the petiole in the evening being thus ascribed to the on-coming of darkness. With regard to the first of these points, I have shown that, even under heliotropic action proper, all dorsi-ventral organs move in a definite plane; and as regards the second, NYCTITROPIC MOVEMENTS 687 it has been shown that the fall of the leaf in the evening is not due to the action of on-coming darkness, but to the cumulative stimulation of the day’s illumination—that is to say, to the action of light as a constant force. It is possible to trace out the gradual evolution of this periodic diurnal movement, taking as the simplest type that of the plagiotropic stem. Under the action of stimulus, which is internally or externally diffused, the lower and more excitable side of such a stem undergoes progressive con- cavity, the lowest position being attained in the evening. At night, however, on removal of the stimulus of light, re- covery takes place by erection of the stem. A diurnal up- and-down movement is thus induced. A similar effect is observed in the petiole of Bzophytum. In the leaf of Mzmosa the action is precisely the same. Here, owing to the direct action, and the positive after- effect of light, the leaf is depressed progressively till evening. At night, however, recovery takes place by an erectile move- ment. This is not due to a passive recovery merely, but is aided by the negative after-effect, consequent on the storage of internal energy by the day’s illumination. By this active internal impulse the leaf attains its highest position some time before dawn. The alternate impulses acting periodi- cally on the leaf are thus: (1) the direct effect of continuous stimulation of light, and (2) the opposite impulse due to internal energy. In addition to these alternate impulses the plant as a whole contributes periodic impulses, which are concordant with the periodic impulses in the leaf. The external tissues of the plant, acted on by light stimulus, contract and drive the water inwards, into the central reservoir. At night a reverse movement of water takes place. As a result of this alter- nation of external stimulus during the day, and internal stimulus during the night, there is a periodic inflow and out- flow, a diminution and increase of tension, and these varia- tions of tension are indicated by the periodic depression and erection of all motile leaves synchronously. 688 PLANT RESPONSE The fact that these periodic forces in leaves and in plant act concordantly causes the periodic movements to be of large amplitude. . The periodic diurnal variations of internal hydrostatic tension are also exhibited by periodic variations in the rate of growth. The curves of the diurnal periodicity of growth and of the nyctitropic movement of the leaf are therefore similar. This forced diurnal vibration, being often repeated, gives rise to periodic after-effects, which persist for a time even on the cessation of the periodically exciting cause. CHAPTER: XETX ON PULSATORY RESPONSE AND SWIMMING MOVEMENTS AS INITIATED AND MODIFIED BY LIGHT .AND OTHER FORMS OF STIMULUS Investigations on the influence of light on the lateral leaflet of Desmodium gyrans : (az) in sub-tonic condition ; (4) in normal tonic condition — Changes induced in existing anisotropy of Desmodium leaflets— Reversal under intense stimulation seen in all forms of response—The swimming movements of ciliated organisms —Fundamental resemblance between the swimming responses and the ordinary heliotropic responses in radial organs—-Phototactic movements: (a) Two natural types of responsive movements ; (4) Responsive movement positive, negative, or intermediate, according to intensity of stimulation—Directive action of light— Thermotaxis--Galvanotaxis—Chemotaxis. WE have already studied in detail the autonomous responses of the leaflets of Desmodium; but there are also other leaves which execute similar pulsatory movements, and we have now to investigate the effect of the stimulus of light on such multiply-responding organs, either in initiating these movements or in modifying them. We have also another instance of such multiple pulsations, in the swimming move- ments of ciliated organisms, of which the causes are at present regarded as very obscure. The treatment of these I include within the present chapter, because I hope to show that they really belong to a class of phenomena essentially similar to the autonomous movements of Desmodium, and that from this point of view all their seeming peculiarities may be very satisfactorily explained. In the first place, I shall take up the question of multiply- responding leaves or leaflets. Concerning the effect of light on these, there is a certain amount of discrepancy of observa- tion. Strasburger, for example, referring to such movements VEY 690 PLANT RESPONSE in the lateral leaflets of Desmodium gyrans, says they are in no way disturbed by variation in the intensity of light. According to Pfeffer, similarly, the autonomous movements of the leaflets of Z7zfolium pratense are not affected by illumination. But Strasburger again states that the leaflets of this plant, on exposure to light, cease their oscilla- tions. Investigations on the influence of light on the lateral leaflet of Desmodium gyrans.—(a) /u sub-tonic condition.— A clearer insight into this subject will, however, be obtained when I describe my experiments on Desmodium gyrans. I have shown that when this plant is in a sub-tonic condition it behaves like an ordinarily responding plant, such as Azophy- tum ; a Single moderate stimulus then evokes a single response, and strong stimulus multiple responses. Thus a leaflet of Des- modium in a state of standstill has multiple response initiated by the incident stimulus of light. The, following record FIG. 275. Initiation of Multiple Response in Lateral Leaflet of Desmodium originally at Standstill Light applied at x and continued till the end of the sixth response, as shown by the thick line. The responses show a staircase increase with increase of absorbed energy. Pulsations persist for a short time even on the cessation of stimulus. (fig. 275) shows this clearly. The leaflet was in a quiescent condition, but during the application of light it exhibited multiple responses, which, owing to the increasing absorption of energy, showed a staircase enhancement of amplitude. On the cessation of light the energy absorbed maintained the pulsation for some time. (6) In normal tonic condition.—When the specimen, how- ever, is in a favourable tonic condition, its pulsations being PULSATORY RESPONSE 691 apparently autonomous, the application of excessive stimulus of strong light often brings about fatigue, and under these cir- cumstances I have obtained two different types of response, according to the particular condition of the tissue. In the first of these, progressive fatigue, under the continued action of light, is shown by the gradual diminution of amplitude of pulsation. If the strong stimulus of light, which causes this fatigue, be applied for a short time only, the pulsation whose amplitude was diminished, again on the cessation of stimulus recovers its first amplitude in a staircase manner ; but if the light be long continued, fatigue becomes so great that the pulsatory movements are brought to a stop, at least for a considerable period. We have seen again that an organ sometimes exhibits fatigue somewhat differently from this, that is to say in a periodic manner. In accordance with this fact the leaflets of Desmodium are sometimes seen to display periodic fatigue—that is to say, the pulses wax and wane in groups, an example of which is seen in fic. 276. Thus we see that in a leaflet at standstill, light, by supplying energy, initiates autonomous movement ; while in an actively pulsating leaflet, strong illumination may bring about such fatigue as to cause cessation of movement At still other times, again, owing to the favourable condition of the tissue, fatigue is slight, and there is no arrest of activity. These facts are sufficient to explain the discrepancy already mentioned, in the observations made by Strasburger and by Pfeffer, on the action of the leaflets of Z7rzfolium pratense under light. Changes induced in existing anisotropy of Desmodium leaflets.—A very interesting consideration arises at this point as to the differential fatigue caused in the anisotropic organ by strong stimulus. We have seen that the lower half of such an organ is the more excitable, hence we can see the probability of fatigue being relatively greater there. Now, it is the naturally greater excitatory contraction of the lower half of the organ which causes the downstroke of the leaflet YeY. 2 692 PLANT RESPONSE to be the quicker; but if stimulus produce greater fatigue of the lower half, we can see that its movement will be made somewhat slower, and the upstroke will then become the relatively quicker of the two. This anticipation finds remarkable verification in the photographic record given in fig. 276. We see there, in the first or normal record, from the fineness and steepness of the upward line, representing the downstroke, that this is the quicker movement of the two; but after the application of strong stimulus of light, at the point marked with an upward Fic. 276. Photographic Record of Autonomous Pulsations in Lateral Leaflet of Desmodium gyrans under Action of Sunlight, showing Periodic Reversals Light applied continuously from arrow onwards. In the first two responses, representing the normal, the downstroke of the leaflet, represented by the up curve, is relatively the quicker. After the application of light, this relation is gradually reversed, till in the fourth and fifth pulses after application it is the upstroke, represented by the down curve, which is pronouncedly the quicker. This reversal is in its turn reversed at the eighth pulsation. arrow, we observe a gradual change, by which the existing normal difference between up and down strokesis first abolished and then reversed. In the next complete response, after the application, we see that the two strokes have become equally rapid. In the next, the downstroke has become distinctly the slower, and this goes on progressively till we see in the fifth of this series a very remarkable degree of difference between the two, the upstroke being now much the quicker. These reversals are found to be recurrent, further on in the record. Such alternate changes of excitability on the two sides we have noticed even in the case of radial organs ; for PULSATORY RESPONSE 693 we have seen that such an organ, when acted on unilaterally by strong light, often exhibits to-and-fro oscillations, due to alternate fatigue of the two sides. We thus find, starting with a Desmodium \eaflet in a state of standstill, that moderate intensity of light initiates normal pulsatory movements, in which the downstroke is quicker than the upstroke; but, in a pulsating leaflet, under intense or long-continued light, these beats are reversed, that is to say the upstroke becomes the quicker. Under the action of continuous light, again, these reversals themselves may become periodic or recurrent. Reversals under intense stimulation seen in all forms of response.—We have already seen that autonomous pulsa- tion is simply ordinary response repeated, owing to excess of energy ; and that the Desmodzum \eaflet is an ordinary aniso- tropic organ, in which response may be initiated by any form of stimulation—thermal, photic, chemical, or electrical. We have just seen, further, in the case of photic stimulus, that the character of the response may be reversed by the in- tensity of the stimulation. Thus the normal response, in which the downstroke is more energetic than the upstroke, may be exchanged for a type of response in which the up- stroke is quicker than the down. This law of reversal of response with varying intensity of stimulation has already been shown to be illustrated in different types of response. For example, in the case of chemical stimulation, we found that the leaf of A/zmmosa when subjected to the action of a dilute solution of sodium chloride gave an erectile response, whereas when the solution was stronger it responded by depression (pp. 551, 552). In the response of growth, again, very dilute and very strong solutions of a given reagent were shown to produce opposite effects. Thus, while dilute solu- tion of sugar accelerated, a very strong solution retarded, the rate of growth (p. 488). In the case, then, of an organ which is capable of multiple response, we find that any form of stimulus is competent to initiate such response, and that, with regard to certain charac- 694 PLANT RESPONSE teristics, such as the relative quickness of the up or down movement, it may be exhibited in opposite ways, according to the intensity of stimulus. The swimming movements of ciliated organisms.—It is the automatic character of ciliary movements which has hitherto rendered them a subject of such great perplexity, for these movements are independent of any nervous system for their initiation or control. The impulses that lead to ciliary motion arise in the cilia themselves. There is, again, some difficulty in understanding the mechanics of these movements. Their automatism may, however, be explained by those considerations which are now familiar to us in similar in- stances—that is to say, initiation of movement by external stimulus, and its maintenance by excess of latent energy. There is, again, the closest resemblance, from a mechanical point of view, between the movements of the cilia and the movements of Desmodium \eaflets. In both cases alike, one of the component strokes is quicker than the other. Again, though in both cases we meet with instances in which the up and down movements take place in the same plane, yet there are also others in which more complicated paths, whether circular or elliptical, are described. The fact that one move- ment is quicker than the other, and that the strokes are lateral, shows that we have in the cilium an anisotropic organ, essentially similar to the pulsating pulvini of the lateral leaflets of Desmodium. The only difference between the two cases lies in the fact that we have in the pulvinus a multicellular, and in the cilium a unicellular, organ. Let us suppose a detached petiole of Desmodium, bearing the lateral leaflets, to be thrown into water contained in a glass trough ; let us further suppose these leaflets to be in a sub-tonic condition, that is to say in a state of standstill. If this vegetable organism be now stimulated by sunlight of moderate intensity, striking it horizontally from one side of the vessel, it will be found that rhythmic excitation is initiated, and that the stroke backwards is much quicker and PHOTOTAXIS 695 more energetic than that forwards. The consequence of this will be, in the vegetable organism as in the case of a man swimming, its forward propulsion. This result depends upon the fact that the lower half of the anisotropic organ is in this case the more excitable. Reversal of the relative activities of the two halves of the dorsi-ventral organ was, however, seen to occur in the case of the leaflets of Desmodium when the intensity of stimulation was very great. Such a reversal, under excessive stimulation, would give rise then to a swimming movement in the opposite direction. We also saw such reversals under continuous stimulation of light becoming periodic (fig. 276). The corresponding swimming response would thus consist of a movement to and fro. Supposing, however, that the excitability of the upper half of the motile organ had been the greater, it is clear that the normal excitatory response would have taken the form of a backward or negative swimming movement. We thus see the possibility of normal responsive swimming movements of two different types, according to the particular half of the anisotropic motile organ which is the more excitable. Taking, again, that type of swimming in which the response is posi- tive or forward, a stronger intensity of stimulation may give rise to a reversal, or negative movement. And from what has already been said, it will be seen that similar responsive movements may also be expected under forms of stimulation other than light. Similarity between swimming responses and the ordinary heliotropic responses of radial organs.—Though at first sight it would appear as if there were no con- nection between the simple responsive curvatures of radial organs and the apparently complicated responsive move- ments of swimming, yet on a closer analysis we shall find that there is little essential difference between the two; for we have seen that growth itself, or growth-curvature, is simply a phenomenon of multiple responsive movements, which, owing to the rapidity of the individual responses, appears continuous. Hence, when, under moderate stimula- 696 PLANT RESPONSE tion, the organ moves towards the light, or exhibits a positive response, this means that the resultant of its multiple move- ments is towards the stimulus, like the resultant movement of the ciliated organism towards light. Similarly, under strong photic stimulation, the negative heliotropic movement of the organ corresponds to the swimming of the ciliated organism away from light. In the intermediate case, again, where the stimulated organ shows no resultant curvature, but oscillates about a mean position, we have an instance which is paralleled, in the case of the ciliated organism, by alter- nate swimming backwards and forwards. Again, just as in the heliotropic response of a radial organ, the minor pulsations by which it is brought about are too rapid and minute to be easily detected, and we can per- ceive only the resultant movement of the organ as a whole, so in the case of the ciliated organism the individual beats cannot easily be perceived, and we infer their presence from the resultant motion of the organism as a whole. Phototactic movements.—From the fundamental de- monstrations which I have already given of the character- istics of multiple response, and its modification by relative variations of contractility, as between the upper and lower halves of the responding organ, it will be found that the multifarious responsive movements of ciliated organisms, under various forms of stimulus of differing intensities, will have been elucidated. (a) Two natural types of responsive movements.~-The two opposite types of movement, positive and negative, which we have now theoretically anticipated, are found to be com- pletely illustrated in the case of swarm-spores under the action of light. Thus, for example, in the case of Botrydium granulatum, they respond by a positive swimming move- ment, or motion towards the light. Again, while certain varieties of Ulothrix exhibit the positive effect, by movement towards light, there is another variety which gives the nega- tive response, by swimming away from it. This opposition of effects is obviously due, as we have anticipated, to a Ea PHOTOTAXIS 697 natural difference of relative excitabilities, as between the upper and lower halves of the anisotropic swimming organ (p. 695). (6) Responsive movements positive, negative, or intermediate, according to intensity of stimulation—TVurning now to the question of different movements in the same organism as modified by the varying intensity of illumination, examples are furnished by the observations of Stahl and Strasburger. These investigators find that the swarm-spores, generally speaking, when the intensity of light is moderate, move towards it, and when stronger, away. In the lateral leaflets of Desmodium, again, under con- tinuous illumination, we have observed recurrent reversals of the direction of the more rapid of the two strokes which con- stitute each individual pulsation. Even this phenomenon finds a curiously exact parallel in the movements of certain swarm-spores of UJlothrix under the continuous action of light, as noticed by Strasburger. These he finds first to retire from the light, then to remain stationary, and again to return towards the light, only then to begin the whole process over again, thus moving to and fro for some time like a pendulum. Directive action of light.—The movement of the ciliated organism, then, whether towards or away from it, is parallel to the direction of incident. light. But a difficult question arises here as to how the organ perceives this direction as it were, and by what mechanism it determines its own course accordingly. At first sight, there appears no reason why rhythmic beats caused by stimulus should not produce propulsive movement in any direction. In this connection, turning to the case of U/othrix, we find the organism pro- vided with symmetrical pairs of cilia. We also know that the excitatory effect of stimulus of light depends upon its angle of incidence. If, then, light strike the two cilia of a given pair asymmetrically, they will undergo unequal excita- tion, causing them to execute a turning movement. Thusa stable condition can only be arrived at when excitation is 698 PLANT RESPONSE equal in the two cilia, and this can clearly happen only when the organism has so orientated itself that its axis is parallel to the direction of the incident rays; and it is evident that in this position the equal beats of both cilia must result either in progressive or in retrogressive motion, parallel to the direc- tion of the incident rays. Thermotaxis.—Thermal stimulation also causes respon- sive movements towards or away from the source of stimula- tion; and these reactions, again, are found to change their signs, with varying intensities of stimulus. Thus, Paramacta swim towards the warmer side of a vessel which is unequally heated, provided the temperature of the warmer side does not exceed 24° C. The response is in this case, then, seen to be positive ; but when the temperature of the heated side is higher than 28° C. the Paramecia are found to swim away, thus exhibiting a negative response. Galvanotaxis.—Similar multiple response finds expres- sion in certain /zfusoria, in swimming movements of posi- tive and negative character. Thus Verworn finds, for example, that under the excitation of an electrical current, Polytoma swims away from the kathode, and Pleuronema towards it. In these galvanotactic movements, also, we meet with the same recurrent reversals with which we are already familiar in the case of the leaflets of Desmodium. In work- ing with Paramecium, Arthur W. Greely! found that after being subjected for about half an hour to a moderate current, the organisms which had previously gathered round the kathode reversed their action, and moved towards the anode, only to dart back immediately, again, towards the kathode. I have demonstrated in Chapter XXXVI. the opposite responsive changes which occur under the action of acids and alkalis respectively. This explains the appropriate modification of galvanotactic response in Paramecium, according as the organism has been reared in an acid or in ' Science has lost a very promising worker in the early death of this investi- gator. His experiments on Paramecium are very valuable and suggestive. eee CHEMOTAXIS 699 an alkaline culture. Thus Greely has found that while alkali-reared Paramecia invariably give the initial response by swimming towards the kathode, the reverse is generally the case with those which have been subjected to acid—that is to say, the latter as a rule begin by swimming towards the anode. Chemotactic movements.—Similar effects are, again, observed under chemical stimulation. The opposite reactions of acids and alkalis which have already been described are the occasion of opposite chemotactic responses. Thus Jennings found Paramecia moving towards, or showing positive reaction to, acids, whereas to alkalis they exhibit negative response, or movement away. Chemotactic reaction, again, is modified by the strength of the solution, that is to say by the intensity of stimulation. The opposite effects of strong and feeble doses (p. 488) are here illustrated by the fact that in many organisms positive response is observed under the action of a weak solution, and negative under a strong. Connected with the subject of chemotaxis is the interesting phenomenon of the response of the antherozoids of ferns and of Selagznella to malic acid, as discovered by Pfeffer. In the response of erowth we found that dilute solutions of sugar gave rise to one kind of response, that is to say to acceleration of growth, and that with very strong solutions, say about Io per cent., the opposite occurred—that is to say, retardation (p. 482). Now, it is found by Pfeffer that whereas the dilute solution of a malate exerted an attractive influence on the antherozoids, a 10 per cent. solution had a repellent effect. SUMMARY A rhythmic vegetable organ, in a state of standstill, has its autonomous movement renewed by a supply of energy from incident light. Too strong an intensity of light may, by causing fatigue, arrest such movement. Or the greater fatigue of the more excitable half of the organ may cause a reversal of the relative rapidities of the up and down beats, 700 PLANT RESPONSE In Desmodium, under the continuous stimulation of strong light, these reversals are often recurrent. The downstroke, which is at first quicker, becomes less quick than the upstroke, and this may be again and again reversed. In a ciliated organism the swimming movements are explicable by similar unequal up and down strokes of the anisotropic cilia. When the downstroke is quicker, the organism propels itself forward. When the upstroke is quicker, there is a movement backwards. Such swimming movements, due to multiple response, are initiated by stimuli of various forms. There are two natural types of these responses : positive movement, or swimming towards, and negative, or away from, stimulus. These are determined by the relative excitabilities of the upper and lower halves of the cilium. Unilateral light exerts a directive action on responsive swimming movements, because the only stable position is one in which pairs of cilia are equally excited. The axis of the organism is thus orientated till parallel with incident light. Strong stimulation of light, for reasons described, causes a reversal of the normal movement. Alternate periodic fatigue causes a responsive movement of the organism to and fro, Under moderate unilateral thermal stimulus the organism exhibits a positive swimming movement, and under stronger a negative movement. Similar responses to stimulus are exhibited under galvanic excitation. The normal response here also undergoes reversal under long-continued stimulation ; since the actions of acids and alkalis are antagonistic, organisms reared under acid and alkali cultures tend to exhibit opposite reactions under galvanotaxis. Under chemical stimulation, swimming organisms exhibit multiple responses. Acid and alkali, owing to their an- tagonistic actions, bring about opposite responses. The same reagent again occasions opposite responses, according to the amount of the dose. Thus antherozoids of ferns are attracted by the dilute solution of a malate, but stronger solutions exert a repellent action. PRT: IX L SURVEY AND CONCLUSION CHAPTER = REVIEW OF RESPONSE, SIMPLE AND MULTIPLE Responsive contraction—Kunchangraphic records—Direct and indirect effects of stimulation—Various forms of responsive expression; (a) Lateral motile response by differential contraction; (4) Suctional response; (c) Growth- response ; (d@) Torsional response ; (¢) Death response ; (/) Thermographs of regional death; (g) Electrical response—Different types of response: (a) Uniform response; (4) Fatigue; (c) Staircase response— Excitability — Conductivity—Polar effects of currents—Multiple response-——Continuity of multiple and autonomous responses—The ascent of sap. WE have now studied in detail the manner in which a plant reacts to the varied forces of its environment, and although its response to them finds modes of expression which appear highly diverse, yet we have found that on analysis these are all reducible to two very simple and well-defined factors, of responsive contraction and expansion. Responsive contraction.—In vegetable cells, as in other protoplasmic bodies, it has been shown that the impact of all external stimulus evokes responsive contraction ; and _ this we found to be true, not in the so-called sensitive plants alone, but in all plants. Taking the simple case of a radial vegetable organ, such as a stem, style, stamen, or other filamentous structure, we find that it undergoes longitudinal contraction under stimulation. Here, then, we have a phe- nomenon which is analogous to the contraction of muscle, The amount of contraction of these ordinary vegetable organs, further, is sometimes very considerable, as we saw in the case of the coronal filament of Passzfora, where it was as great as 20 per cent. of the original length. Such responsive contraction takes place, moreover, under all forms of stimulation, mechanical, thermal, electrical, photic, and chemical ; and we have found that all the various move- 704 PLANT RESPONSE ments of plants which are seen in nature, under the action of external stimulus, are but different expressions of a single fundamental response by contraction. Kunchangraphic records.—By taking advantage of this responsive contraction, we are able to study all the physio- logical modifications induced in the vegetable tissue by various reagents, with as great ease and certainty as similar phenomena can be studied in animal muscle, using myographic records. By such study, again, we are once more led to see how misleading has been the superficial distinction be- tween sensitive and non-sensitive plants, since the latter, or so-called ordinary, plants also exhibit contraction under stimulation. Direct and indirect effects of stimulation.—The living organism is thus a delicately responding machine, whose responsive movements are brought about by external stimulus ; but this complex machinery has also the power of holding part of the energy of the external stimulating shock latent, for a longer or shorter time, so that part only may find immediate expression, while the rest is stored up as internal energy to be given out after the lapse of an intervening period. These two factors, of external stimulus and internal energy, again, induce opposite effects, of contraction and expansion respectively. And the infinite multiplicity of responsive processes in the life-cycle of the plant is brought about by their mutual play. That the combination of these two elements in varying degrees of each, finding expression in different ways, creates a tangle which would at first sight appear inextricable, can be easily understood. And it was the bewilder= ment which this fact imposed upon the observer, that drove us to postulate the existence of an unknown and indefinable vital force, whose mysterious working was to be held to account for the occurrence of all those phenomena that we were otherwise unable to explain. It is possible, however, as we have found, going back step by step, to trace out the different expressions of these two distinct factors, of external stimulus and internal energy, and EE REVIEW OF RESPONSE, SIMPLE AND MULTIPLE 705 to show, moreover, how the latter may be derived from the former. This may be clearly and easily seen by taking a simple case, in which we excite the stem by the application of external stimulus, on a zone a few centimetres below its upper end. As the direct effect of this stimulation, a contraction takes place in that zone. By this contraction an active expulsion of water, with local negative turgidity-variation, is brought about, and the expelled water is driven outwards in both directions from the excited zone. Following the course of the water which is thus forced upwards, we have found that it produces an increase of turgidity, or positive turgidity- variation, with consequent distension or expansion of all the cells above the stimulated area. Work is thus performed on these cells, in consequence of external stimulus, by which their latent energy is increased. The energy of the stimulus applied in one has thus been _ conveyed to another region, hydraulically, there to give rise to an effect of increased turgidity and expansion, which was designated as the indirect effect. Thus the effect of external stimulus is seen to be twofold—namely, first by its direct action to induce local contraction, and secondly by its indirect effect, of increasing the internal energy, to bring about an expansion. The expressions of direct and indirect stimula- tion are thus seen to be opposite in character, and we have seen how they find opposite modes of expression in the case of each form of response. Various forms of responsive expression.—The next » question to be passed in review is that of the various modes in which the response of the plant was seen to find expression ; and here we found various responsive phenomena which are characteristic of life, and apparently entirely unrelated, to be ultimately dependent upon this fundamental inter-action between, on the one hand, the contraction due to external stimulus, and on the other the expansion due to internal energy. (a) Lateral motile response by differential contraction.— Taking first the responsive mechanical movement of motile Zz 706 PLANT RESPONSE organs, we find that its gradual evolution can be traced from the simplest case, namely, the longitudinal response of a radial organ. In such a radial organ, if one side be rendered in any way the less excitable—say by the unilateral applica- tion of cold or anesthetics, or by the fatigue induced by long-continued unilateral stimulation—we have an induced anisotropy. On now subjecting the organ to diffuse stimula- tion, the relatively more excited side will undergo the greater contraction, and the response will thus be by that movement which results from the concavity of the more excited. We find many instances again of the various stages through which this anisotropy passes before it culminates in the dorsi-ventral pulvinus ; thus, for example, in a spirally formed tendril, where concavity has been induced by the unilateral excitation of that side, the concave surface is less excitable than the convex; and such a tendril, when diffusely excited by strong electrical stimulus, exhibits the excitatory effect by extraordinary writhing movements due to the relatively greater excitation and concavity of the originally convex side (p. 92). A plagiotropic stem, again, whose upper side is fatigued by the action of sunlight, exhibits on diffuse stimulation a downward movement, due to the greater excitatory contraction of the more excitable lower half (p. 86). The phenomenon of azfferential response, then, whose various preliminary stages we have thus traced, comes to its greatest perfection in the dorsi-ventral pulvinus of such plants as MWzmosa, for we have found that in the last-named organ the characteristic responsive movement is not brought about by the action of excitable cells restricted to the lower half. That the upper half also is excitable is shown by the fact that on applying to it localised stimulus, say of light, its cells contract and raise the leaf (p.631). The fall of the leaf, on the application of diffuse stimulation, is thus the result of the greater excitability of the lower. This fall, again, is not due to mere flaccidity caused by the ex- pulsion of water from the excited organ, but to the actual differential contraction of the lower half, whose activity may REVIEW OF RESPONSE, SIMPLE AND MULTIPLE 707 be gauged by the tension it can be made to exert on a spiral spring (p. 26). As regards mechanical response, then, a single law—-that response is always by concavity of the more excited side—will be found universally applicable. In a dorsi-ventral organ, like the pulvinus of J/zmosa, we have seen that response to diffuse stimulation is always by the contraction of the more excitable lower half. The same law, however, is also applicable even in the case of a radial organ excited unilaterally, for here the side acted on is relatively the more excited, and response is by concavity of that side. A radial organ, acted upon from one side, undergoes concavity of that side, and consequent movement towards the stimulus. The same is true of pulvinated organs, when either the upper or lower half is acted upon locally by stimulus. As in radial organs, then, so also here, under these circumstances, we obtain instances of the directive action of stimulus ; but when stimulus is either internally or externally diffused, we obtain from a dorsi-ventral pulvinus the greater contraction of the more excitable half, and movement is thus made, for anatomical reasons, to occur in the direction at right angles to the plane which separates the two aniso- tropic halves. Even in A7zmosa, the contraction of the pulvinus itself is not very great, but the contractile movement is highly magnified by the attached petiolar index. When a radial organ is diffusely stimulated there is no lateral movement at all, and from this fact it has been erroneously inferred that ordinary plants are not sensitive. In fact, however, every plant is sensitive, and exhibits contractile response, which is shown, in the case of radial organs, by longitudinal con- traction under diffuse stimulus, and by lateral response under unilateral stimulus. The direct effect of stimulus on the pulvinus of MWzmosa, causing a negative turgidity-variation of the organ, finds appropriate expression in the responsive movement of fall. But we have seen that the expression of the indirect effect of ZZ2 708 PLANT RESPONSE stimulus is opposite in character to that of the direct effect. The indirect effect of stimulus, or the increase of internal energy, thus results in an erectile response, as seen in the erection of the leaf in A/zmosa, or Biophytum, when their internal energy is in any way enhanced, say by rise of temperature (p. 400). Taking all kinds of response, it will be found universally true that if the increase of internal energy give rise to one form of expression, the impact of external stimulus evokes the opposite. () Suctional response—It has been shown that con- tractile response gives rise to a forward impulsion of water ; hence by the excitatory contraction of the root-cells the movement of water upwards is initiated. When such con- tractile movement is not single, but repeated or multiple, continuous propulsion of water is maintained. The rate of this propulsion therefore affords us a means of measuring the rhythmic activity of the plant-tissue. (c) Growth response—This pumping-in of water causes the transmission of energy to the distant growing region, and as by this the internal energy of the plant is increased, it finds expression there in a pulsatory expansion, which is the movement of growth. Following each pulse of expansion, there is a recovery which is incomplete, owing to fixation of srowth-material. The resultant growth is thus the irreversible effect of the entire process. This growth-movement is another expression of that indirect effect of stimulation which we have already considered, by which a distant excited point gives rise to a progressive train of waves of positive turgidity- variation. The action of these waves may be seen not only in the movement of expansion at the growing point, but also by the erectile response of an intervening motile organ. This will be understood from the following diagram of an artificial plant (fig. 277), which shows how contractile action at the base, giving rise to an hydraulic wave, causes two different expressions of (a) motile response of the lateral organ, or leaf, and (4) growth-expansion of the terminal growing point. The pulvinus of this artificial motile organ consists of an ee REVIEW OF RESPONSE, SIMPLE AND MULTIPLE 709 india-rubber tubing, the lower half of which is thinner, and therefore more expanding or responsive, than the upper. The upper end of the tube, representing the end of the plant, is closed over with a thin elastic strip of india-rubber. When now there is contractile action at the base, the pulse of increased hydraulic pressure thus induced is found to bring about a practically simultaneous erection of the artificial lateral leaf L, and an expansion or bulging outwards of the terminal yielding body G. The analogy in the case of the latter would be still more perfect if we imagined it covered, instead of with india-rubber, with some plastic substance which would set quickly on elongation or expansion, thus representing the permanent erowth-effect. From this we see the connection between growth and those other responses with which Fic. 277. Model of an Arti- we are already familiar. The only ficial Plant difference between the responsive S¥dden compression of yindia- ; : rubber bulb, Rk, causes expansion of pulvinated organs and hydraulic wave upwards, : One : : downward movement being this responsive expansion of growth prevented by valve V. lies in the fact that in the former This wave of oe . . ressure causes erection oO recovery is perfect, and in the latter ae leaf, L, and the expan- imperfect. It must also be borne ce m the terminal septum in mind that growth may be initiated locally at the growing region if the sum total of energy, directly or indirectly supplied to it, be above par. We further see in the case given that growth-response is not, strictly speaking, ‘of its own accord’ or spontaneous ; for the energy of a definite stimulus, causing contractile movement at the base, is transmitted hydraulically, and performs the work of growth. It would be as accurate to describe the work done by hydraulic machinery as spontaneous, ignoring the energy that had set the pump in action, as to call growth FLO PLANT RESPONSE spontaneous, without recognising or tracing that energy which in the form of stimulus must have been supplied to some part of the plant machinery. We have seen that a plant in a state of growth-standstill has its activity renewed when a stimulus is applied to the distant root, and that when the amount thus supplied is exhausted, the activity ceases. In growth, then, which is regarded as so characteristic a phenomenon of vital action, we see the law of the conservation of energy holding good, as in an ordinary inorganic system. The plant thus expresses the absorbed energy, by a responsive expan- sion, either of erection or of increased rate of growth, according to the particular organ of response which is concerned, both of these constituting cases of work done by internal energy, or indirect effect of stimulus ; but where the responding organ is directly excited by external stimulus, we obtain a contractile response of the organ, with expulsion of water, or negative turgidity-variation. The hydraulic cur- rent in our model is now reversed, and opposite responsive movements take place, by the fall of growth below the normal rate, and by depression of the leaf. If now we take a balanced record of growth, the impact of a uniform series of external stimuli will be found to give rise to a series of responses by depression of the rate, followed by recovery, exactly similar to those records of depressions of the leaf, with recoveries, which are obtained from pulvinated organs. We may also see the expression of external stimulus and cessation of stimulus, in the appropriate periodic variations of growth, and movements of motile organs, which occur under the stimulating action of daylight, and the withdrawal of such stimulus at night. Thus in the daytime we see a response consisting of depression of the rate of growth, which corresponds to the depression of the leaf, say of JZzmosa, and at night a recovery, or enhancement of growth, and erection of the motile organ. The daily periodic curves obtained of this growth-variation and responsive mechanical movement are very similar. REVIEW OF RESPONSE, SIMPLE AND MULTIPLE 711 (ad) Torsional response-—Another interesting type of response occurs when an anisotropic or dorsi-ventral organ is stimulated laterally. Under such conditions, a responsive torsion is induced, by which the less excitable side is made to face the external stimulus. The extent of the response depends on the intensity of stimulus, and the differential excitability of the anisotropic organ. In dorsi-ventral organs, the plane of anisotropy is fixed. But in certain climbing plants, this plane revolves in a positive or negative direction, and under the internal activity of growth, an autonomous torsional movement is thus observed. The opposition of the effects of internal energy and external stimulus is here seen, when such an organ is uni-laterally acted on, say by light. The autonomous torsional movement is then found to be retarded, or even reversed. (e) Death response —There is another curious phenomenon of response, which takes place at a certain definite point. When an organ is gradually raised in temperature, the internal energy is increased, and the organ exhibits a re- sponsive expansion, if radial by elongation, or if pulvinated by erection ; but when the death-point is reached, a sudden and irreversible molecular change takes place, attended by an excitatory contraction. In the curve of thermo-mechanical response we here find a sharply defined point of reversal, which affords us an exact index of the death-point. This death-point is very definite in plant-organs under normal conditions ; in phanerogamous plants it is very near 60° C. Physiological modification of the tissue, moreover, may be gauged by the transposition of the otherwise definite death- point (p. 185). (f) Thermographs of regional death.—Just as there is a definite point of reversal in the thermo-mechanical curve, so there is also a point of discoloration which is, under standard conditions, at a determinate interval from the death-point. A particular region, physiologically changed, may thus be thermally ‘developed, and made to exhibit as a thermo- sraph, a picture of localised physiological variation (p. 184). 712 PLANT RESPONSE (g) Electrical response—We have, lastly, to consider briefly the electrical mode of responsive expression. The excitatory contraction, with negative turgidity-variation, of a vegetable, as of an animal tissue, is accompanied by an electrical variation of galvanometric negativity. The in- direct effect of stimulus with its positive turgidity-variation, moreover, has also a concomitant electrical expression of galvanometric positivity (p. 37). Different types of response: (a) Unzform response.— These various expressions of response are brought about by the fundamental molecular change induced by stimulus. When stimulus is moderate, and sufficient time allowed for recovery from molecular distortion to the original condition, a series of responses to uniform stimuli will be uniform ; but if very strong stimulus be applied, recovery will only be completed after a long interval. (6) Fatigue-——If successive stimuli be applied before complete recovery has taken place, the successive responses will exhibit diminution, or fatigue. Under strong and long- continued stimulation the plant-tissue exhibits, as in the case of tetanised muscle, a fatigue-reversal—that is to say, the contracted tissue passes into what is apparently its original expanded condition; but the difference between the normal condition and the condition of fatigue-reversal is seen in the fact that, while the former is sensitive to fresh stimulation, the latter is insensitive. The fatigued tissue, however, resumes its original excitability after a period of rest. This fatigue-reversal explains the erection of the Mimosa \eaf under continuous stimulation (p. 110). We observe similar fatigue-reversals, even in inorganic substances like india-rubber, where the normal contraction under thermal stimulation passes into relaxation under the long-continued action of such stimulation; and the india-rubber becomes sensitive again only after a sufficient period of rest (p. 120). In connection with this, we sometimes meet with the very curious case of alternate or periodic fatigue, both in living and in inorganic substances. The simplest type of this REVIEW OF RESPONSE, SIMPLE AND MULTIPLE 713 occurs when, under uniform stimuli, responses are alternately large and small. These alternations sometimes show them- selves in groups. Under continuous stimulation, again, this periodic fatigue exhibits itself by response of an oscillatory character. (c) Staircase response-—The tissue is sometimes in a relatively sluggish condition, and by the absorption of stimulus the molecular mobility is gradually increased. The effect of this is seen in the amplitude of successive responses, increasing in a staircase manner. It is usually supposed that response is brought about by a chemical run-down of energy, of an explosive character. The external stimulus is thus supposed to act as it were on a trigger, to release the latent energy. The response is hence assumed to be disproportionately larger than the stimulus. That this cannot, however, hold good in all cases is clear; for the tissue is often found to absorb a certain proportion of the incident stimulus, the immediate expression of response being thus disproportionately smaller than stimulus. The energy of the system is now found, instead of being lowered, to be raised above par. The internal energy thus held latent is sometimes seen, as in the case of strongly stimulated Bzophytum, to find expression later by multiple response. In the case of growth-response, again, it is a variable fraction of incident stimulus that finds immediate expression in the direct effect of retardation of growth, whereas the absorbed component gives rise to the subsequent responsive effect of an enhanced rate of growth. Referring to the former of these as the direct, and to the latter as the indirect, effect of stimulus, it is found that the sum of the two remains approximately constant. Below the optimum tonic condition it is found that the indirect effect is relatively the larger, but near the optimum this relation is reversed, and the direct effect is the larger. In a sub-tonic condition stimulus produces little or no direct effect, it being utilised to produce the indirect effect of enhanced growth. At the optimum, on the other 714 PLANT RESPONSE hand, practically the whole of the incident energy is expressed in direct response, there being little or no absorbed element (p. 460). A similar series of considerations may be applied to the response of mature pulvinated organs. In this case, however, the indirect effect of stimulus will find expression in an enhancement of the rate of recovery of the organ. It is, however, difficult always to discriminate with certainty between the natural and an enhanced rate of recovery ; but on turning to growth-response we find that, by using the balanced method of record, it is easy to distinguish between the direct’ and indirect effects of stimulus, since these are here shown by curves in opposite directions. This method moreover affords us some means of measuring - the relative magnitudes of the two factors in the response. Excitability.—It is interesting to find that an agency which induces a variation of excitability produces a similar modification of all the different forms of response. In this way the long-continued application of cold has the effect of lessening excitability, and the response of a motile organ is thus found to be temporarily diminished or abolished. At the moment of application, however, owing to the fact that any sudden variation of environment acts as a stimulus, its effect is the induction of an excitatory movement. These phenomena are repeated with curious exactness in the case of -suctional response. On the application of ice-cold water to the root, the immediate effect is a transient exaltation of suction, followed later by depression and arrest (p. 375). In growth-response, also, growth is diminished or arrested by this agency. Another effect of the moderate application of cold is to induce a molecular sluggishness by which the latent period is increased. A moderate rise of temperature, on the other hand, increasing the molecular mobility, has the contrary effect, of reducing the latent period. Anesthetics, again, induce a diminution or abolition of excitability, as is seen by their effect on the various forms of response. REVIEW OF RESPONSE, SIMPLE AND MULTIPLE 715 The excitability of a tissue which has not recovered fully from previous strong stimulation is found to be impaired. The fatigue-effect only disappears after the lapse of a period of rest. If, then, the resting intervals between successive stimuli be gradually shortened, the motile responses will be found to be progressively diminished, and a time arrives when the succeeding stimulus evokes no response, the tissue having become as it were refractory. The minimum interval during which the tissue remains thus irresponsive is known as the refractory period. In Azophytum, under normal condi- tions, this period is about ten seconds in duration (p. 273). Conductivity.—It is usually supposed that the trans- mission of the excitatory effect, as seen in sensitive plants like Jtmosa, is merely the transmission of a_hydro- mechanical disturbance, and therefore unlike the transmis- sion of the excitatory effect in animal tissues. It has, however, been shown that this is not the case, for here, as in the animal, transmission of excitation takes place by the propagation of protoplasmic changes. It was shown further that the hydrostatic disturbance was transmitted with a relatively great rapidity, whereas the true excitatory effect has a slower and definite velocity, characteristic of the particular specimen. This velocity, moreover, is found to be modified, and that in a manner precisely similar, by all those agencies which modify the velocity of transmission of excita- tion in animal tissues. Thus, cold, anesthetics, and fatigue are all influences which reduce the velocity of transmission. As an example of this, we saw, in a certain specimen of Biophytum, in which the normal velocity was 3°8 mm. per second, that slight cooling reduced it to I°3 mm. per second, or almost to one-third. Conversely, the raising of the temperature from 30°C. to 37°C. increased the velocity from 3°77 to 9'I mm. per second. Strong stimulus is found to be conducted further and more rapidly than feeble or moderate. It is found in the case of animal tissues, again, that on account of its physiologically depressing effect, the anode acts as a block to the transmission of excitation; and the 716 PLANT RESPONSE same statement holds good in the case of the plant. The passage of mere hydro-mechanical disturbance could not have been affected in any way by the anode. Nor is this transmission of excitation confined to sensi- tive plants alone. The fact that it occurs in all plants alike I have been able to demonstrate by various other methods, in using which we are rendered independent of the mo- tile indications afforded by lateral leaflets. Thus by the Electrotactile Method we are enabled to detect, in any zone of the plant, the moment of arrival of the state of excitation from a distant point (p. 259). The Electro- motive Method, again, displays the moment of arrival of the wave by the induced galvanometric negativity of the point (p. 261). The Chemical Method, again, shows the arrival of the wave of excitation by the projection of a dense precipitate, produced in a suitable solution (p. 255). All these different methods give us results which are in mutual agreement. As showing how ill founded is the common distinction as between sensitive and ordinary plants, it was demonstrated that the velocity of transmission in some of the latter is greater than in the former. Thus in Fzcus religiosa the velocity was determined at 94 mm. per second, whereas in the ‘sensitive’ Veptunza oleracea it was only I‘I mm. per second. The velocity of transmission of the excitatory impulse in plants is found, again, to be of the same order of magnitude as in the nerves of lower animals (p. 252). I have shown further that excitation is transmitted in the plant in both directions. It is, however, interesting to note that, generally speaking, the facility of this trans- mission is greater centrifugally—that is to say, in the direction of the ascent of sap—than centripetally. Thus, for example, in a petiole of Azophytum, while the centripetal velocity was 1°8 mm. per second, the centrifugal velocity was 3 27 mm. per second. Since conduction of stimulus takes place by transmission of protoplasmic change, such change is naturally conducted REVIEW OF RESPONSE, SIMPLE AND MULTIPLE 717 most easily along those paths in which there is most proto- plasmic continuity. Parenchymatous tissues, in which the cells are divided from each other by more or less complete septa, are thus relatively inefficient as conductors of the excitatory effect to a distance. Hence, the lamina of the leaf does not transmit its local excitation to any distance ; but tissues which contain fibro-vascular elements, and which are thus characterised by a greater protoplasmic continuity, are therefore better conductors of excitation. Thus stems, petioles, and peduncles are better conductors than the laminz of leaves, and parallel-veined leaves, again, are better in this respect than reticulated. As regards stems, petioles, and peduncles, moreover, the conducting power is greater longitudinally than transversely. In the peduncle of Musa, for example, I find the conductivity lengthwise to be three times as great as that crosswise. In consequence of this difference, it is found that the transmitted excitatory effect of a stimulus unilaterally applied is greater on the same than on the opposite side. This explains the appear- ance of responsive concavity at a distance from the point of stimulation, but on the same side. It is to be remembered that, owing to the fact that this conduction of the effect of stimulus is an excitatory process, we find that in autumn and winter, when the physiological excitability is low, the conducting power of the tissue is also very much reduced. Various degrees of conductivity are possessed by different tissues, and the distance to which the excitatory effect is conducted depends not only on the conducting power, but also on the strength and duration of stimulus. Thus, while in a feebly conducting tissue the effect of moderate stimu- lation is not transmitted to any distance, strong and long- continued stimulation is transmitted to a certain extent. Even in a better conducting tissue the excitatory effect of a moderate stimulus, on account of gradual enfeeblement, can only reach up to a certain distance. It is to be borne in mind that, while no tissue is absolutely non-conducting, 718 PLANT RESPONSE neither is any a perfect conductor, the difference between extreme examples being one only of degree. The true excitatory effect, whether due to direct or trans- mitted excitation, consists, as has been shown, of contraction, with concomitant negative turgidity-variation. The result of this contraction and concomitant expulsion of water is, however, the sending out of a wave of positive turgidity- variation. Thus, up to the point reached by the true excitatory effect, we obtain contraction, with negative turgidity - variation; and beyond this point, a _ positive turgidity-variation, with consequent expansion. This latter effect we have designated as the indirect effect of stimulus. It is thus seen that, whereas the direct effect of unilateral stimulus is a concavity, its indirect effect is a convexity. We have also seen that it is possible by electrical means to determine whether it is the direct or indirect effect of stimu- lus which has in any given instance reached a point from a distance ; for the indirect effect of stimulus, with its positive turgidity-variation, is always attended by galvanometric posi- tivity, whereas the true excitatory effect with its negative tur- gidity-variation is characterised by galvanometric negativity. A tissue may conduct without exhibiting any motile indication of its state of excitation. With reference to this it is to be borne in mind that certain advantageous circum- stances are necessary for the display of motile response ; for since the fall of an excited leaf, such as that of MWzmosa, takes place in consequence of the expulsion of water, it follows that when this is in any way impeded, as by over- turgidity of the tissue, there may be excitation without any responsive movement ; for this reason, the leaflets of Azo- phytum,in the morning, when they are most tense, are not so sensitive as later in the day. Motile excitability is as a rule found to be abolished earlier than conductivity ; hence a strong stimulus may be conducted through a region which exhibits, through narcotisation, no motile excitability (p. 229). Polar effects of currents.—Another observation by which the fundamental identity of excitatory phenomena in SS REVIEW OF RESPONSE, SIMPLE AND MULTIPLE 719 the animal and vegetable may be seen, lies in the respective effects induced at anode and kathode; for example, em- ploying the leaflets of Bzophytum-as experimental specimens, and using a moderate E.M.F., we find that the excitatory depression of the leaflets takes place at the kathode at make, and at the anode at break. The antagonistic effects of anode and kathode are further seen in the fact that while the kathode-make excites, the anode-make depresses. It is owing to this latter fact that an excitatory wave is blocked during transit at an anodic area (p. 233). Another very interesting difference between anode and kathode, both at make and break, is seen in the fact that at make, while an induced contraction takes place at the kathode, an induced expansion occurs at the anode; at break both these effects are reversed, there being now an expan- sion at the kathode, and contraction at the anode. Expansion at make, moreover, attains its maximum in a short time, while the kathodic contraction is relatively strong and _per- sistent. These fundamental effects find appropriate expression in the response of growth. Thus, the unilateral application of the anode induces expansion, acceleration of growth, and resultant convexity, while the effect of the kathode is to induce contraction, retardation of growth, and resultant con- cavity (p. 558). Owing to the fact that kathodic action is stronger than anodic, a feeble or moderate current flowing through the soil exerts a predominant excitatory action on the roots, by which the suctional activity of the plant is increased. The result is an increased rate of growth of the plant, which is independent of the direction of the current through the soil (p. 560). The normal polar effects which have been described take place under the action of a moderate electromotive force. When this is excessively high, however, the normal effects are, or tend to be, reversed. In this reversal there appear to be two stages—the A stage and the B stage. In the A stage both anode and kathode excite at make ; but in the B stage, 720 PLANT RESPONSE under a still higher E.M.F., there is a complete reversal, inasmuch as the anode here excites at make, and the kathode at break. This reversal, further, is facilitated by fatigue of the tissue (p. 215). These polar effects may also, as I have shown, be demon- strated in the case of animal tissues by means of the glow- response of the firefly. An excitatory reaction is here shown by an increase of the intensity of luminescence, and a depressing reaction by its diminution. Multiple response.—When response is observed by means of the electromotive or electrotactile method, we obtain a single response to a single moderate stimulus ; but on the application of strong stimulus a multiple series of responses is found to be evoked. In the case of the retina, similarly, a single intense stimulation by light gives rise to recurrent visual impulses. In the same way, in the leaf of Bzophytum, while a single moderate stimulus gives rise to a single mechanical response, a strong stimulus gives rise to a multiple series of responses. In this case certain other peculiarities may also be observed ; for instance, a certain minimal intensity of stimulus induces the maximal mechanical response, which is not increased by any increase of intensity of the stimulus. The excess of such stimulus is held latent by the tissue for the time being, to find subsequent expression as rhythmic multiple response. These multiple responses are evoked by all forms of stimulation, mechanical, thermal, chemical, photic, and electrical. . In this respect, of the minimally effective stimulus inducing maximal response, we have an important point of resem- blance between the actions of a rhythmic plant-tissue and the cardiac muscle of the animal. Both, again, are charac- terised by the exhibition of a long refractory period, which is an expression of fatigue, or temporary loss of excitability after excitatory discharge. The periodic oscillation of excit- ability which is thus induced, imparts a rhythmic character to the mechanical expression of the excess of stimulus which is held latent in the tissue. REVIEW OF RESPONSE, SIMPLE AND MULTIPLE 721 The sum total of the energy derived by the plant from the various stimuli of its environment determines what is known as its tonic condition. Continuity of multiple and autonomous responses.— There is no line of demarcation between the phenomena of multiple and autonomous response. When the latent or internal energy of the plant is above par, it finds expression in the form of multiple response, which is apparently auto- matic. Taking the typical case of a multiply-responding plant which is furnished by Azophytum, we find, on supplying it with excess of energy, by maintaining it at the tempera- ture of say 35° C., and thus exalting its tonic condition, that it displays autonomous response. Conversely, when the tonic condition of an autonomously responding plant, such as Desmodium, is in any way reduced, by reason of low tem- perature, unfavourable season, or other circumstances, it becomes converted into an ordinarily-responding plant like Biophytum. A single moderate stimulus now gives rise to a single response, and a strong stimulus to multiple responses. It is in accordance with this, that a Desmodium leaflet in a state of temporary standstill has its multiple or autonomous response renewed by any circumstance, or combination of circumstances, which sufficiently enhances the internal energ of the plant. Amongst such circumstances are: (1) The action of light ; (2) favourable temperature ; (3) the presence of stimulating chemical substances ; (4) an increase of internal hydrostatic pressure. The energy which expresses itself in pulsatory move- ments, then, may be derived by the plant either directly from immediate external sources ; or from the excess of such energy already accumulated and held latent in the tissue, aided by the incidence of external stimulation ; or from an excessive accumulation of such latent energy alone. Thus there is, strictly speaking, no such thing as automatism, for only when acted upon by stimulus can a living tissue give responsive indications. The impact of an external stimulus may give rise to immediate response, or it may be held 3 A 22 PLANT RESPONSE latent, in whole or in part, for subsequent expression. ‘ Inner stimuli’ are simply external stimuli absorbed previously, and held latent. A plant or an animal is thus an accumulator, which is constantly storing up energy from external sources ; and in the case of the plant, its suctional activity, determining the ascent of sap, its growth, and its spontaneous motile indications, are some of the principal forms in which this accumulated energy finds expression. The ascent of sap.—The ascent of sap has been shown to be due to a multiple excitatory reaction of the plant- tissue, the movement of the water being a secondary effect of the rhythmic activity. The excitatory nature of the phenomenon has been demonstrated by the fact that various agencies which induce increase or diminution of excitability, have also the effect of bringing about an enhanced or diminished rate of suction, above or below the normal. The effects induced by these agencies, together with their time- relations, can be easily and accurately recorded, as has been shown, by means of the Balanced Shoshungraph. Vhe transient excitation due to a sudden application of cold, and the abolition of excitation under its prolonged application, are seen in a transient enhancement of suction, followed by arrest. The excitatory effect of the application of hot water, again, is shown by an enhanced rate of suction. Poisonous chemical reagents arrest suction quickly in specimens where, owing to a less favourable tonic condition, the power of resistance is low, and slowly in other cases. As the tissue of the plant exhibits this suctional activity throughout its length, the local death of a given portion, by scalding or by poison, would not necessarily arrest the suction of the entire plant. Such an arrest can only occur definitely when the entire plant is killed. The internal energy, on which the activity of suction depends, may fall so much below par as to bring it toa standstill ; but the activity is renewed on the application of fresh stimulus. That the ascent of sap is not fundamentally due to i i — REVIEW OF RESPONSE, SIMPLE AND MULTIPLE 723 transpiration from the leaves is seen from the fact that in a saturated atmosphere it continues to take place. That it is not, again, fundamentally due to the osmotic action of the concentrated cell sap in the leaves is seen from the fact that the ascent continues to take place on the removal of leaves. It is seen again from the further fact that under favourable circumstances, on the application of an osmotically strong solution of sodium chloride to the root, the cell sap, instead of being withdrawn by osmotic action, is made, by the excita- tory effect of the salt, to ascend more vigorously. The ascent of sap is thus an excitatory phenomenon, and its uni-directioned flow is due to the graduated passage from point to point of the co-ordinated excitatory reaction, propelling water forward. This rhythmic excitation is ini- tiated in the intact plant at its root, by the stimulus of contact with soil, the friction of the growing organ against rough surfaces, the excessive turgidity caused by the absorp- tion of water, and possibly by the chemical stimulus of substances present in the soil. Inthe case of cut branches placed in water, the excessive turgidity at the cut end initiates rhythmic activity, which drives the water upwards ; but if such a branch be placed upside down, with its foliage in water, the now turgid anatomically upper end becomes the seat of excitation, and the direction of the flow of sap is reversed. The connection between the conduction of stimulus and conduction of water is seen from the fact that the movement of water takes place preferentially along those channels which are also good conductors of excitation. Hence it is transported more easily along the plant than across it; and while the movement is possible either upwards or down- wards, yet it is quicker in the upward direction, which is also preferentially the direction of conduction of stimulus. The same movement of water which is produced by the co-ordinated rhythmic activity of cells throughout the plant appears either as suctional or as pressure movement, accord- ing to the point of view which we adopt. When the removal : 3A2 724 PLANT RESPONSE of water from the plant is in any way arrested, a positive pressure is produced, owing to its excessive accumulation. Similarly, when loss is greater than supply, the pressure will be negative. The ascent of sap, primarily due to cellular activity, may be secondarily aided by evaporation from the leaves, and by the osmotic action of the concentrated cell sap there. Owing to the distribution of unequally active cells, an irregular variation of pressure may be induced in the stem. The excitatory movement may be transmitted to a distance by conduction, or there may be conduction by ‘relays.’ An isolated mass of highly excitable tissue may thus be excited de novo. The excretion of water and of nectar are phe- nomena of cellular activity, analogous to that which brings about the ascent of sap. The translocation of food-material is also probably due, at least in part, to excitatory reaction. The internal activity of the plant, causing increase of turgidity, may be detected mechanically by that erection of the leaf which is characteristic of the positive turgidity- variation. Any increase of internal activity is exhibited in dorsi-ventral organs, such as the petioles of Azmosa, Biophy- tum, and Artocarpus, by the erection of the leaf. Thus, when the internal energy of the plant is increased by a rise of temperature, the leaves become erected. Conversely, under the action of cold, on account of the diminution of the latent energy, the opposite effect, or droop, is induced. This ex- plains the drooping of various leaves during frost, and their subsequent erection, when brought into a warmer atmosphere. =? 4 ee CHAT FER sicl > RESPONSIVE GROWTH-CURVATURES IN PLANTS Longitudinal growth and its variations—Effect of temperature on growth—Re- sponsive growth-curvature under unilateral stimulation :—-1. Direct unilateral stimulus on the responding organ: (a) Positive response under moderate stimu- lation ;_(/) Intermediate or neutral response ; (c) Negative response ; (@) Dorsi- ventral positive response; (¢) Dorsi-ventral response which may become negative —2. Indirect effect of unilateral stimulation : (7) Negative response ; (2) Positive response—Responsive action under stimulus of gravity—Helio- tropic action in radial organs—Heliotropic action in plagiotropic and dorsi- ventral organs — Phototactic movements—Nyctitropic movements. WE shall next pass in review the responsive growth- curvatures induced in plants by various agencies, and shall then in the following chapter consider at some length the extended range of those similarities which exist as between the physiological responses of plant and of animal tissues. Longitudinal growth and its variations.—It was shown by means of the highly magnified continuous record which was obtained with the ordinary and Balanced Crescographs, that growth was a phenomenon of multiple response ; and it was further shown that these multiple responses of growth exhibited the same characteristics as had previously been observed in the multiple motile responses of Azophytum and Desmodium. ‘ach of the constituent responses consisted of a sudden elongation due to a pulse of increased turgidity, followed by an incomplete recovery. The _ irreversible crowth-effect consisted of the difference between this elon- gation and its recovery. These pulses of positive turgidity- variation were mainly due to excitatory reactions occurring about the zone of growth, which delivered from within, upon 726 PLANT RESPONSE the plastic material of that zone, repeated hydrostatic blows. The consequent expansive response was thus the indirect effect of stimulation. It is thus the internal energy, ultimately derived from external stimulus, that gives rise to those rhythmic activities by which the pulsations of growth are maintained. When the sum total of the latent stimulating factors that determine the tonic condition is below par, there is an arrest of the multiple response of growth, corresponding to the similar arrest of multiple motile response in Desmodium. Ina plant in which growth is at standstill, it may be renewed by a fresh supply of energy. Thus, if hot water be applied to the root of such a plant, energy is hydraulically transmitted to the srowing region, and there re-initiates growth. If moderate stimulus be thus imparted, the responsive srowth-movement persists for a short time, and then comes to a standstill, to be again renewed by a fresh supply. Again, the movement of growth being due to the indirect effect of stimulus, we might renew or accelerate it by apply- ing stimulus, say, on the stem or its top, at such a distance from the growing region that the direct excitatory effect would not be transmitted to it. Stimulus applied directly on the growing region would, however, by its true excitatory effect, induce contraction and retardation of growth. The longitudinal growth thus described takes place in a strictly radial organ. If the organ, however, be bilateral, instead of radial, it will exhibit lateral oscillation, owing to the alternate growth of the two sides. Or growth may pro- ceed in a spiral line, giving rise to circular or elliptical move- ments. A very good example of the last is afforded by the torsional growth-movements of climbing plants. These various circummutating autonomous movements of growth, passing from regular movements in a circle, through ellipses, to a straight line, are exactly paralleled by different examples of autonomous mechanical responses in Desimo-_ dium, where also we find circular, elliptical, and rectilinear movements, RESPONSIVE GROWTH-CURVATURES IN PLANTS 727 Effect of temperature on growth.—That growth is an excitatory phenomenon is seen, again, in the fact that it is increased by any circumstance that tends to increase excit- ability. Thus, for example, in the case of most tropical phanerogamous plants, it is found that responsive excitatory contraction is greatest at a temperature of about 35° C.; and this is also found to be the optimum temperature, at which the natural rate of growth is at its maximum. I have described a method of obtaining a THERMO- CRESCENT CURVE for the determination of the various rates of growth which correspond to different temperatures. The continuous record thus obtained in the course of about half an hour affords us not only the rate of growth at any tem- perature, but also a means of determining its optimum and maximum points. The optimum temperature may also be determined, with an accuracy within one-tenth of a degree, by means of the Balanced Crescographic record. The results obtained by all the different methods employed are found to concur. The optimum point is thus shown, under normal conditions, to be very constant (p. 451). It may be said here that in the case also of plants which exhibit tor- sional growth-response, the rate of torsional movement is greatest at this optimum point. The arrest of growth which occurs at the maximum tem- perature does not appear to be due to any cessation of activity as brought on by rigor; for we found in a record taken from a seedling of Balsam at 44°C. that at this temperature the constituent growth-pulsations had actually become more frequent than before, the resultant abolition of growth being due to the fact that response and recovery were now equal. It was likewise shown that the apparent arrest of the pulsatory movements of Desmodium at certain high temperatures was not due to the cessation of activity, but that at such temperatures the pulsations had become more frequent and very minute (p. 431). The fact that at the maximum temperature growth is not arrested by rigor receives curious illustration, again, when the application of 728 PLANT RESPONSE doses of poison at such a temperature brings about, at least temporarily, a renewal of resultant growth (p. 487). Another important point in the effect of temperature has already been alluded to. It has been shown that a piant below the optimum temperature, being in proportionately sub-tonic condition, will to a very great extent, or even entirely, hold the incident stimulus latent, thus increasing its own latent energy. In this sub-tonic condition, then, the stimulus induces little direct contractile effect, but is utilised to induce the indirect acceleration of growth. At the optimum temperature, however, almost the whole of the incident stimulus finds expression in direct contractile response, there being now little or no absorbed com- ponent; and beyond the optimum, the tissue not only possesses little or no power of holding stimulus latent, but its receptivity also appears to undergo great diminution (p. 461). Responsive growth-curvature under unilateral stimu- lation.—I have shown that the response of a growing is not essentially different from that of a pulvinated organ. The direct effect of unilateral stimulation gives rise in both cases alike to negative turgidity-variation, with consequent con- . cavity of the side acted upon; and the indirect effect, on the other hand, consequent on the unilateral stimulation of a distant point, gives rise, in both cases alike, to a positive turgidity-variation, or convexity of the same side of the responding region. This fact was demonstrated in the case of Mimosa by applying stimulus: (1) near the motile organ, in which case we obtained the direct effect by fall of the leaf; and (2) at a considerable distance, when the indirect effect gave rise to the erection of the leaf (p. 531). It was found, however, that when the stimulus applied at a distance was very strong and long-continued, true excitation was ultimately transmitted by conduction, inducing excitatory contraction, with fall of the leaf. In growth-curvatures, similarly, we obtain responsive movements appropriately due either to the direct or indirect E: 2 % 3 } 2 F RESPONSIVE GROWTH-CURVATURES IN PLANTS 729 effect of stimulus. “These have been shown to be classified as follows : 1. Direct unilateral stimulus on the responding organ: (a) Poszttive response under moderate stimulation.— The proximal, by the direct action of stimulus, contracted ; and the distal, by the indirect action of stimulus, expanded. The result was a concavity of the proximal, and convexity of the distal, conspiring to bring about movement. towards stimulus. (6) Intermedtate or neutral response.—Though the trans- verse conductivity of a tissue may be feeble, yet under somewhat strong stimulation the true excitatory effect is transversely conducted from proximal to distal. The result is that when the two opposite sides are equally excited, there is a neutralisation, or disappearance of responsive curvature; or, by alternate fatigue of the two sides again, the organ may be made to oscillate to and fro about a more or less mean position. (c) Negative response—When stimulus is very strong and long continued, we obtain not only the transverse conduction of effect, but also temporary induction of anisotropy of the organ. The proximal side is now, owing to fatigue brought about by the direct impact of excessive stimulus, the less excitable ; and the internally diffused stimulus, causing greater contraction of the more excitable, induces concavity of the distal, or a negative responsive movement. Besides this we have organs which are characterised by a permanent anisotropy or dorsi-ventrality, and we then obtain two classes of effects, according as the transverse conductivity is very feeble or moderately strong. Owing to the dorsi-ventral structure, the responsive movement can only take place at right angles to the plane which separates the anisotropic halves of the organ. These effects are the Same in growing organs, such as plagiotropic shoots and dorsi-ventral petioles, and in mature dorsi-ventral organs, such as pulvini. (2) Dorst-ventral positive response.—When the transverse 730 PLANT RESPONSE conductivity is feeble, the stimulus remains localised on the side of the organ acted upon. Thus the stimulation of either upper or lower side induces a positive response, or movement towards stimulus. (e) Dorst-ventral response which may become negative.— When the transverse conductivity of the organ is con- siderable, and the excitability of the lower half relatively great, then the strong stimulation of the upper side will, by internal diffusion, cause contraction in, and concavity of, the more excitable lower. The responsive movement will then be negative, or away from stimulus; but feeble or moderate stimulation of the upper half, not being transmitted to the lower half, causes a positive response. Direct excitation of the more excitable lower half will always give rise to a movement towards stimulus, or positive response. 2. Indirect effect of unilateral stimulation: (a) Negative response—When moderate stimulus is unilaterally applied at a distance from the responding organ, it is the indirect effect that is transmitted to that region, causing convexity of the same side, with consequent movement away from stimulus, or negative response. This is very well illustrated when the tip of either shoot or root is sub- jected to moderate unilateral stimulation. (6) Positive response——But when the unilateral stimulus at the distant point is strong or long continued, the excitatory effect is transmitted by conduction, and induces a contraction and concavity of the same side, resulting in a movement towards stimulus, or positive response. From what has been said it will be understood that moderate unilateral stimulation of the tip of root or shoot induces negative, and excessive stimulation positive, while between these two extreme cases there may be intermediate or neutral, response of the responding region. These effects are induced by stimulation of all forms, and it is thus clear that there is no specific sensitiveness of the dorsi-ventral as distinguished from the radial organ, nor is there any polar difference between the response of root or ——— <= ~~ a RESPONSIVE GROWTH-CURVATURES IN PLANTS 731 shoot, the tips of both organs behaving alike. The one universal law which applies in every.case is, that the direct effect of stimulus is to induce contraction, and its indirect effect to cause expansion. On taking a general survey of the responsive movements which are induced by the unilateral action of stimulus, we find that moderate stimulation of the growing region induces a positive movement. Or negative movement, again, may be induced in either of two ways—that is to say, by moderate stimulation of the tip, or by very strong stimulation of the growing region. Responsive action under stimulus of gravity.—I have shown that the unilateral application of pressure of particles is efficient to cause responsive contraction. An experiment was described in which it was shown that the unilateral pressure of magnetically attracted particles would induce concavity of the side acted on (p. 497). The weight of statolithic particles may thus be the efficient cause of stimulation by gravity. It is to be borne in mind, however, that stimulation caused by such means as the weight-effect of these minute particles can only be moderate. We have therefore in the case of geotropic stimulation to deal only with the direct and indirect effects of unilateral stimulus of moderate intensity. In the case of the stem the growing region is directly stimulated. A horizontally laid stem thus curves upwards to meet the lines of force, or rays of gravity, just as it would bend upwards under heliotropic action to meet the rays of incident light. It is supposed that the curvature of the stem under gravity is mainly due to an active growth of the convex side; but I have shown that it is due, on the contrary, to an excitatory response, which consists, like all other forms of response to external stimulus, of a contraction. The active element in the in- duced responsive curvature is thus the contraction of the upper side of the organ, aided subsidiarily by that expansion of the under side which is brought about by the indirect effect of stimulus on the distal. That this is the case is seen 732 PLANT RESPONSE from the fact that, on localised cooling of the upper side, the movement of the organ in response to gravity is abolished, whereas cooling of the lower side has little or no effect on the responsive movement. This experiment incidentally sup- ports the view that it is the inner tangential wall of the cells which is relatively effective in responding to the stimulus of gravity. In turning to the geotropic response of the root, on the other hand, we find that it is the distant tip which is the perceptive region for gravitational stimulus. Hence it is only the indirect effect of stimulus which acts on the responding growing region. But we have seen that moderate stimulation of the tip, by any form of stimulus whatsoever, always induces a movement at the responding region, of opposite sign to that which is the result of direct stimulation, and from this the opposite geotropic responses of shoot and root follow as a matter of course. This fact entirely negatives the assumption that shoot and root are possessed of any polar difference of sensibility, or that any specific geotropic sensibility has been evolved in the radicle for the advantage of the plant. Heliotropic action in radial organs.—We shall find similarly, in studying the various movements of the plant in response to heliotropic stimulus, that, diverse as they seem, they arecharacterised by an underlying unity, being in fact but so many expressions of the universal law that response takes place by the contraction and concavity of the more excited. The fundamental effect of light was demonstrated by showing that, in a growing organ, diffuse stimulation induces a contraction and retardation of the rate of growth. This was also shown to be true of all other forms of stimulation, including those of thermal and electrical radiation. The incidence of radiation may, it is true, cause a rise of tem- perature ; and this would, as we know, have the effect of enhancing the rate of growth. In order, therefore, to discriminate the effect of radiation as such from that of temperature, an experiment was described in which the circumstances were so arranged that no rise of temperature ES er errr RESPONSIVE GROWTH-CURVATURES IN PLANTS 733 could take place while the effect induced by radiation as such was being observed. Under these crucial conditions it was demonstrated that the effect of radiation is to induce responsive contraction. Under moderate unilateral stimulus of light, as in the case of gravity, two definite and distinct effects were observed, according as stimulus was applied directly on the responding region or on the distant tip. In the former case we obtained a positive, and in the latter a negative, responsive movement. Up to this point, then, the actions of light and of gravitation are parallel in their effects—that is to say, the positive heliotropic movement of the stem corresponds to the so-called negative geotropic movement of the same organ ; and the negative heliotropic movement of the root to its so-called positive geotropic. Looked at in relation to the direction of stimulus, however, it may be said that the response which is commonly known as ‘negative geotropic’ is actually positive, and wice versa; for, accepting the theory of statolithic or hydrostatic pressure as to the effective cause of stimulation, the direction of the excitatory pressure is in the direction of the lines of force of gravity. In a stem laid horizontally, then, and acted on by vertical lines of gravitational force, or by vertical rays of light, we obtain the same directive response to these similar directive stimuli, by the bending upwards of the organ to meet the rays, or the lines of force. Some confusion is therefore inevitable when one of these responses is designated as positive, and the other as negative, for the essential similarity of the two is here masked by the use of directly opposite terms. This difficulty might perhaps be Overcome by naming the normal responsive movement of the stem as fosttive phototropic and positive gravitropic, or pro-gravitropic, and that of the root as negative phototropic and negative gravitropic or anti-gravitropte. We next turn to the differences between the effects of helio- tropic and geotropic action. Such differences arise from the two facts that : (1) only in the root is the region of the percep- tion of gravitational stimulus separated from that of response ; 734 PLANT RESPONSE and that (2) geotropic stimulus is always of moderate in- tensity. As regards the first of these two differences, it has been shown that, on applying unilateral heliotropic stimulus of moderate intensity to the tip of the shoot, we obtained the same negative response of indirect stimulation as is given by the root-tip. In the case of geotropic stimulus, however, there can be no phenomenon corresponding to this, inasmuch as in the stem the statolithic particles appear to be diffused, instead of being localised at the tip. The second point of difference between the two responses arises from the fact that heliotropic stimulus may be of any degree of intensity. Hence the direct excitatory effect of strong unilateral stimula- tion of the root-tip may in the case of light be transmitted to the growing region, and there induce a positive response, or movement towards stimulus. This accounts for the fact that while roots in general give one kind of gravitational response of so-called positive sign (but really negative), some roots give negative response to light, and others positive. Turning next to the direct action of unilateral heliotropic stimulus on the growing region, we find, as explained in the summary of responsive action in general (p. 535), that the effect is modified by the intensity of stimulus, by the transverse conductivity of the organ, and by its existing anisotropy. Thus in the case of a radial organ, such as the hypocotyl of Szzapzs, moderate stimulus, its effect remaining localised on the proximal side, has been shown to evoke a positive responsive movement. Stronger or long-continued stimulus, reaching the distal side by transverse conduction, neutralises this first effect, and the organ thus remains at right angles to the incident light, or in a dia-heliotropic position, apparently unaffected by it. In other instances, again, owing to the alternate excitation of the two sides, the organ may oscillate to and fro about a mean position. With still stronger stimulus, however, an anisotropy is induced, by which the proximal side becomes, through fatigue, the less excitable, and the internally diffused stimulus causes greater contraction and resultant concavity of the distal side ; that is ————————— ee rLUr RESPONSIVE GROWTH-CURVATURES IN PLANTS 735 to say, a negative response. . It was thus made clear that the three types of response—positive, negative, and dia-helio- tropic—are not due to three different specific sensibilities. It has been pointed out, further, that these considerations explain why it happens in many cases that, while moderate stimulation induces a considerable responsive movement, stronger stimulation, instead of increasing this, actually neutralises it. It is due, as we have seen, to the transverse conduction of stimulus by the tissue, that the positive effect is counteracted or reversed. This explanation has been shown to ~ account satisfactorily for various cases apparently anomalous. Certain tendrils are regarded as heliotropically insensi- tive. For example, the tendril of Passzfora when acted on by sunlight shows little or no responsive movement. On artificially diminishing the transverse conduction, however, by the application of cold, I have shown that it exhibits the ordinary positive responsive movement. The tendril of Vitis, again, which is supposed to be endowed with a specific sensibility of negative character, has. also been shown to exhibit the normal positive response under light of moderate intensity. The modifications of transverse con- ductivity which are brought about by age and season, with their consequent appropriate variations of response, are seen in Trop@olum. A very young tissue, as a general rule, owing to the fact that the fibro-vascular elements are not fully developed, is a bad conductor of stimulus, which therefore remains localised at the point of application. Hence young plants exhibit movements of positive response, whereas older plants, owing to transverse conduction, with its effect of neutralisation, appear to be little affected by light. In con- nection with this it must also be borne in mind that the power of contraction declines with age. The characteristic effect of season, again, results from the fact that the con- ducting power of a tissue is at its feeblest in autumn and winter, and correspondingly greater in spring and summer. In autumn, therefore, stimulus remains localised, and Z7vo- peolum and Ivy during that season respond to heliotropic 736 PLANT RESPONSE stimulus by positive curvature; whereas in summer strong unilateral stimulation is transversely conducted, and induces negative responsive curvature. Heliotropic response in plagiotropic and dorsi-ventral organs.—We have seen that negative response is brought about in a radial organ by induced anisotropy, and transverse conduction of stimulus. Effects fundamentally similar are seen in organs which are characterised by a natural aniso- tropy. A connecting link between this transient induced anisotropy of radial organs, and the permanent anisotropy of a dorsi-ventral pulvinus, is afforded by plagiotropic stems, in which anisotropy has become more or less permanent, owing to the long-continued unilateral action of vertical light. Two different types of response are exhibited by anisotropic organs, depending on their transverse conductivity and on the relative excitabilities of their two sides. In the first of these, transverse conductivity being feeble, vertical illumina- tion remains localised, and induces positive response. This is the true explanation of the so-cailed diurnal sleep, with upward folding of the leaflets, of Rodznza, Erythrina tndica, and Clitoria ternatea (p. 629). In the second type, the stimulus of vertical illumination is transmitted to the more excitable distal side, inducing concavity of that side, and consequent negative response. The different stages of this effect are well seen in J/zimosa, when the stimulus of light acts on the dorsal or upper side of the pulvinus. Here the immediate effect is a positive response or erection of the leaf ;. and as the stimulus percolates to the distal side, this effect is neutralised and converted into an increasingly negative response. The greatest degree of negativity or fall in nature is thus attained by the cumulative action of the whole day’s illumination. Such is the response which is characteristic of the second type. The action of strong vertical light in such cases induces movement downwards. And this is seen in plagiotropic stems like those of Cucurbita and /pomea ; in the thallus of Marchantia, and the midribs of various leaves ; and in the so-called diurnal sleep, with downward folding of the RESPONSIVE GROWTH-CURVATURES IN PLANTS 737 leaflets, of such leaves as those of Oxalis, Biophytum, and Averrhoa. Inthe heliotropic responses of ordinary leaves, again, we have exactly similar classes of phenomena. In these cases it has been shown that there is no specific dia- heliotropic sensitiveness, such as that by which the upper surface of the leaf was supposed to place itself at right angles to the light, for the purpose of absorbing the largest possible amount of stimulus. It was shown, moreover, that with regard to that response by which the ultimate position of the leaf is determined, the lamina was not the perceptive organ. In ordinary, as in pulvinated, leaves we find response to be of two extreme types, connected by innumerable gradations. First, we have leaves like that of Mangifera indica, in which on account of the feeble conductivity of the pulvinoid vertical illumination induces a positive response, or move- ment upwards ; and as an example of the second type we saw that’ negative response, or movement downwards, was given by the leaf of Artocarpus under vertical light. These responsive movements induced by light, although, generally speaking, predominant, are modified by the presence of other subsidiary factors, which all contribute in various degrees to bring about the variety of attitudes ultimately assumed by the leaves. These subsidiary factors were enumerated as: (1) the epinastic or hyponastic tendency ; (2) the general turgescent condition of the plant; (3) the characteristic limits of flexibility of the motile organ; and (4) the differential geotropic excitability of the organ. When a petiole is acted on laterally by light a torsion is induced, by which the upper surface of the leaf is made to face the incident stimulus. It has been shown that this movement is not due to any specific dia-heliotropic sensibility ; for any form of lateral stimulation, say chemical or thermal, will induce a similar response by torsion, the result being always that the less excitable surface is made to face the stimulus. Similar effects are also observed in compound strips made of such unequally contractile substances as ebonite and india- rubber. Los) to 738 PLANT RESPONSE The fact that it is the differential excitability of the organ which under lateral stimulation causes this torsional movement was further demonstrated, when the difference was artificially increased by the local application of chloro- form to the upper half of the pulvinus. The torsional response was then found to take place, with a corresponding enhancement of rate, in the same direction as before. But when this natural difference of excitability was reversed, by the abolition through local application of chloroform of the excitability of the lower half of the pulvinus, the direction of the responsive torsion was found to undergo reversal. By carrying out a similar series of experiments, with special reference to the lateral action of gravitational stimulus on a dorsi-ventral organ, it was shown that such an. organ as a whole exhibited neither a positive nor a negative, but a differential geotropic action. The investiga- tion showed that the upper half of a pulvinus was less excitable than the lower half under geotropic stimulus. An artificial increase of the existing difference between the excitabilities of the two halves enhanced the rate of the normal torsional response, and the reversal of these natural excitabilities reversed the direction of the torsional response to geotropic stimulus (p. 664). Phototactic movements.—A leaflet of Desmodium in a state of standstill resumes its pulsatory beats when stimu- lated by light. Owing to the anisotropy of the motile organs, one half of the beat is more rapid than the other. Too strong an intensity of light, however, by causing greater fatigue of the more excitable half of the organ, may cause a reversal of the relative rapidities of the up and down beats. In Desmodium, under the continuous stimulation of strong light, these reversals are often recurrent. The downstroke, which was at first the quicker, becomes less quick than the upstroke, and this reversal may take place again and again, in alternation with its opposite. These effects, seen in a pair of anisotropic motile organs in Desmodium, afford an explanation of the swimming movements of certain ciliated RESPONSIVE GROWTH-CURVATURES IN PLANTS 739 organisms. These swimming movements are brought about by the rhythmic beats of the anisotropic cilia under unilateral stimulus of light, either the up or down stroke of each such beat being relatively quicker than the other. For reasons which have been explained, moderate stimulation, initiating these rhythmic responses, causes movement in one direction, and stronger stimulation movement in the opposite direction ; or, as in the case of Desmodium \eaflets, there may be recurrent reversals, causing alternate progressions or retro- gressions to and fro. Similar forms of response with similar variations are brought about by forms of stimulation other than light; there are thus thermotactic, galvanotactic, and chemotactic swimming movements. Nyctitropic movement.—The nyctitropic movement has been shown to be the result of heliotropic action, the fall of the leaf of W/zmosa at evening being due not to the action of on-coming darkness, but to the cumulative stimulus of the whole day’s illumination. Taking this plant as the type, it was shown that the diurnal movement was caused by the action of two different periodic factors, namely: (1) the differential effect of light on the pulvinus itself during the day, alternating with the cessation of stimulus at night ; and (2) a periodic inflow and outflow of water, which takes place in the plant as a whole by the recurrent action of light and darkness. By the first of these factors the leaf is progres- sively depressed during the day, the reverse process taking place during the night, as a result of natural recovery, aided by the conserved internal energy which gives an impulse opposite to that of external stimulus. These two periodic factors, of the effect on the pulvinus itself, and of that on the plant as a whole, act concordantly, and give rise to periodic movements of the leaf which are of large amplitude. Such forced diurnal vibrations, by long repetition, give rise to periodic after-effects which persist for a time, even on the cessation of the periodically exciting cause. CHAPTER’ Ior ON PHYSIOLOGICAL RESPONSE, AND ITS CONTINUITY IN PLANT AND ANIMAL Vitalism—Fundamental unity of physiological response in plant and animal— Theory of Darwin—Variation as induced by external forces. WE have reviewed, in the last two chapters, the various phenomena of plant response. We shall now turn our atten- tion to the consideration of irritability, or the capacity of responding to stimulus, in general. Vitalism.—We have seen that when a tissue is rendered molecularly sluggish by any physical means, such as cooling, its irritability is found to be temporarily abolished. Irritability is thus ultimately due to molecular responsiveness, and ex- citatory response is brought about by the molecular derange- ment consequent on stimulus, with the subsequent self- recovery. We have seen further that the state of excitation is exhibited, either by a mechanical or by the electrical mode of response, and that even where mechanical indications are not available, the electrical sign of excitation is unfailing. We have also seen briefly in the course of the present work, and I have demonstrated in full elsewhere,' the fact that similar excitatory response is given, even by inorganic matter, under stimulation ; and in such cases also we have been able to observe and record not only the phenomenon of response itself, but also its numerous appropriate modifications under varying conditions. Thus fatigue brings about diminution of inorganic, as of organic response. Amongst chemical reagents, again, some induce exaltation and others depres- sion ; and many so-called poisons act here, as in the case ' Bose, Response in the Living and Non-Living, 1902. Eee heen eee PHYSIOLOGICAL RESPONSE IN PLANT AND ANIMAL 741 of the plant or animal, by inducing the abolition of response. Irritability or molecular responsiveness, therefore, must be regarded not as characteristic of organic substances alone, but as the universal property of matter. In the case of what is commonly known as the living, we have merely higher complexities, with greater instabilities, of molecular structure. External stimulus is here liable to induce greater derange- ment, and the irreversible molecular change known as death takes place the more easily, the more highly organised the complexus may be. Bacteria, for example, will survive con- ditions which would immediately prove fatal to more com- plex organisms. In studying the responsive phenomena of living organisms, therefore, we must fix our attention on their molecular aspect, and try to follow out the physico-chemical changes which are consequent on the molecular derangement induced by stimu- lus; and we are more likely to succeed in obtaining a grow- ing insight into the various phenomena of life, when we approach the subject from this point of view, than when we permit ourselves to evade each difficulty as it arises by referring it to the inexplicable action of a mystical vital force. Physical and mechanical considerations at first appear to us to be inadequate to the explanation of the complex movements of the living machine, just as the similarly com- plex movements of a wind-motor, connected with a hidden electrical apparatus, would at first sight be inexplicable to an inexperienced observer (fig. 278). Let such an observer be brought face to face for the first time with such a windmill. Its movements under the action of wind will arouse his wonder, and this will be increased when some- times even in the absence of wind he sees the vanes revolv- ing still, but now in an opposite direction. He may again notice oscillatory rotations, now in one way and then in the other. Failing to find any rational explanation of these movements in this first stage of his inquiry, he will be driven to attribute them to an unknown power, whose characteristic it is to manifest itself by such erratic actions, 742 PLANT RESPONSE now in one direction and again in the opposite, and whose mystery lies mainly in this caprice. But the observer, in the course of his further inquiry, finds that the vanes, whose rotation under the impact of the external stimulus of wind first attracted his attention, are but a part of a complex machine, the interior of which had been hidden from his view. He finds that the energy supplied from outside is being transformed by a dynamo inside, and stored upinanaccumulator. When the external force is not acting, the reverse move- ment is caused by the internal energy thus stored up. This very movement, being apparently without a Fic. 278. Diagrammatic Representa- cause, he would formerly tion of a Windmill with Attached | dactormared t Dynamo, D, and Accumulator, s NAVE esignate as auto- Wind acting on vanes, Vv, from right, matic. When the stored- represented by arrow (<), causes up energy is exhausted, responsive rotation in direction a opposite to that taken by the hands the seemingly autonomous of a watch. This external energy — movement comes to a stand- also causes electrical storage. On the cessation of the wind the still, and only by the accumulator begins to part with its stored-up energy, and, the dynamo now acting as a motor, causes a stimulus, causing renewed responsive rotation of the vanes in : the other direction, as shown by the storage, can it be resumed, ge Nig At a given moment, more- over, the responsive move- accession of fresh external ment of the vanes is determined by the opposing actions of the external and internal factors. As long as the wind is sufficiently strong, movement takes place in one direction, and when there is a pause the internal energy begins to find expression, by causing movement in the opposite direction. If the circumstances were such that the rise of the wind were synchronous with day, and its fall with night, PHYSIOLOGICAL RESPONSE IN PLANT AND ANIMAL 743 the windmill, with its alternate movements, would afford a very excellent illustration of the alternate day and night phases in the nyctitropic movement of the plant. Now, with regard to the living machine, similarly, a full insight into its action can only be obtained if we are able to disentangle the two opposite factors, of internal energy and external stimulus, and follow them into their responsive expressions, at the same time recognising that the principle of the conservation of exergy must hold good in the living system as in the non-living. External energy acting on the plant performs work oz it, a part of this incident energy being taken and held latent ; and, in virtue of the latent energy so conserved, work is performed dy the plant. Of this internal work, besides the potential chemical energy which is accumu- lated, the maintenance of suction, growth, and autonomous movements may be cited as examples. Fundamental unity of physiological response in plant and animal.—Having now seen the intimate connection which exists between the physical and the physiological, and having also seen that the molecular response of the inorganic is not altogether different from that of the plant, it remains only to glance at the physiological continuity of response as between plant and animal; and here we shall find that there is hardly any phenomenon of irritability, observed in the case of animal tissues, which is not also to be discovered in some simple form in the case of the plant. These resem- blances, moreover, are so numerous and so detailed as to lead us inevitably to the conclusion that we have to deal in the two cases with a single identical phenomenon. In the longitudinal response of a radial vegetable organ we have seen how similar is responsive contraction in animal and plant; and that this similarity extends even to charac- teristic details is seen when we compare the records of the Kunchangraph in the case of the plant, with those of the Myograph in that of the animal. The two are alike in the exhibition of a latent period, which is prolonged by cold and reduced by warmth. The staircase increase of the respon- 744 PLANT RESPONSE sive effect, its diminution by fatigue, and the induction of tetanus under rapidly succeeding stimuli, in either case corresponds to a like phenomenon in the other. Fatigue- relaxations, moreover, under strong and_ long-continued stimulation, are the same in both. And, turning to the electrical mode of response, we find that the excitatory condition of a tissue is indicated by its induced galvano- metric negativity, whether the tissue be animal or vegetable ; and further, similar physiological modifications, as induced by the action of various external agents, are manifested by similar changes in the two cases in the electrical response. As by the nerves of the animal, so also by certain con- ducting channels in the plant-tissue, the state of excitation is, in the two cases alike, transmitted to a distance; and this conduction takes place in both by propagation of proto- plasmic changes. In both alike, cold reduces, and warmth accelerates, the velocity of the transmission. In both alike, the stronger the stimulus, the greater is the velocity with which it is transmitted. In both, this velocity is diminished with fatigue. Anzsthetics cause the temporary abolition of conduction in both. The anode, again, blocks the trans- mission of the excitatory wave in both. And, lastly, the velocity of the transmission of excitation in the plant is com- parable to that of its transmission in the nerves of some of the lower animals. Thus, in a certain specimen of the sensi- tive AWimosa, excitation was found to be transmitted with a velocity of 14 mm. per second ; and in the ordinary plant Ficus religiosa this velocity was determined at 94 mm. per second ; while in the nerve of Azodon a value of 10 mm. per second ‘has been recorded. If, then, the characteristic of nerve be to conduct excitation, it must be admitted that the plant, like the animal, is provided with a nervous system. © In the matter of the excitation induced by the electrical current, there is an equally remarkable similarity of effects in animal and vegetable. Under normal conditions in both cases, the kathode excites at make and the anode at break ; PHYSIOLOGICAL RESPONSE IN PLANT AND ANIMAL 745 and that reversal of these normal effects which is liable to occur under fatigue, or under excessively strong electro- motive force, is the same in the one case as in the other. In addition, moreover, to the contractile effects of kathode-make and anode-break, the plant exhibits a responsive expansion at anode-make and kathode-break, which is also seen in animal tissues. Turning next to such tissues as are characterised by the property of rhythmicity in a marked manner, we have seen that this effect as exhibited by the plant cannot be distin- guished from that in the animal. The rhythmic tissues of the plants Bzophytum and Desmodium are characterised by the possession of relatively long refractory periods, a peculiarity which also marks the rhythmic cardiac muscle of the animal. The response of rhythmic tissues in both plant and animal is found to be on the ‘all or none’ principle. In both alike the rhythmic tissue is incapable of tetanus; and in both, when at standstill, a single moderate stimulus gives rise to a single response, and stronger stimulus to a multiple series of responses. In Desmodium, again, as in the cardiac muscle, increased internal hydrostatic pressure renews pulsation in a tissue at standstill. In rhythmic tissues, again, whether animal or vegetable, under favourable tonic conditions, per- sistent pulsatory movements take place which are apparently automatic ; and these rhythmic pulses are found to exhibit the same types of cyclic variation in the plant as in the animal. The effects of temperature on both are exactly the same— ‘that is to say, its rise increases the frequency and diminishes the amplitude of pulsation. And still more striking, finally, is the identity of the modifications induced by drugs in the rhythmic responses of animal and vegetable. But this unity of rhythmic responses in plant and animal is not merely a question of their fundamental characteristics. They sometimes appear also to subserve functions somewhat similar. Thus the rhythmic cardiac tissue of the animal maintains the circulation, and we have seen that the rhyth- mic tissue of the plant maintains the ascent of the sap. 740 PLANT RESPONSE From such considerations it may perhaps appear not very far-fetched to regard a plant as possessed of a diffuse heart. With special reference to the effect of drugs on plant and ~ animal tissues, we find the identity of phenomena similarly impressive. This is exemplified in both cases by their action not only on ordinary contractile tissues, but on rhythmic tissues also. Thus it is only necessary to mention such facts as that anesthetics, like the vapours of ether and chloroform, induce a transient abolition of excitability, with abolition of response, in both animal and vegetable ; that this excitability, with its concomitant response, is gradually restored in either case on blowing off the applied vapour ; and that poisonous reagents, on the other hand, induce a permanent abolition of all response. The action of these and other chemical agents has already been described in some detail. In the case of rhythmic response, again, a like parallelism was found to exist, as between the effects of drugs on plant and animal tissues respectively ; and this parallelism was further shown to extend through a wide range of phenomena. A remarkable instance was seen in the antagonistic effects on responsive rhythmic tissues of the actions of acid and alkali. Acid, when applied to cardiac muscle, induces a diastolic standstill, whereas the effect of alkali is exactly the opposite, a standstill, namely, of systolic contraction; and the standstill induced by either of these is found to be counteracted by the application of the other. Now, I have shown that the effects of acid and alkali on the rhythmic tissues of Desmodium are similarly antagonistic. Thus, dilute hydrochloric acid induced arrest of pulsation in the diastolic, or relaxed, position, in the motile organ of Desmodium ; whereas, with solution of sodium hydrate, the induced arrest was of systolic contraction. Moreover, when arrest in its own particular position had been brought about by either of these reagents, its effect was neutralised by the application of the other (p. 353). The same effects were found again curiously reproduced in the case of the autonomous response of growth. Here, the application of acid induced an arrest eee LL ae PHYSIOLOGICAL RESPONSE IN PLANT AND ANIMAL 747 of growth, but only after an abnormal relaxation or expan- sion. Alkali, on the other hand, induced arrest, but after contraction. And, finally, the arrest induced by one was counteracted by the effect of the application of the other (p. 484). These facts, and others which have already been fully described, afford a conclusive demonstration of the essential unity of the physiological effects of drugs on plant and animal tissues. The existence of such a unity having been established, it is evident that much may be gathered from investigations carried out on plants, as to the obscure question known to medical practice as the modification of the effect of drugs by individual constitutions ; for while a given individual will succumb quickly to the action of a certain poison, another, as is well known, will throw: off its influence and survive. Again, a particular dose of a given drug may have the effect of producing excitation in one case, and in another profound depression. The effect on a tissue of any given reagent, then, does not merely represent the action of that reagent as such, but is further determined also by the reacting power of the responding tissue itself. And this reacting power is modified by what is known as the individual constitution of the organism. Thus no result can be definitely predicted of a reagent, unless we have a precise knowledge of the action of the same drug on various definite constitutions. This problem, of the variations induced in the effects of drugs by the different reacting powers of different constitutions, may now be attacked, therefore, through the study of the plant, in which, as I have shown, it is possible to induce known differences of constitution by artificial means. It was shown, for example, in one case that, while a 5 per cent. solution of the poisonous reagent, copper sulphate, produced an immediate depression, quickly followed by death, another similar plant, whose tonic condition had been raised to the optimum, was found to withstand the action of this poison for a considerable time, the immediate effect being an actual exaltation of its response. The opposite effects of the 748 PLANT RESPONSE samé dose on different constitutions were shown, again, in the fact that, while a I per cent. solution of copper sulphate caused depression, and subsequent death, of a plant under normal tonic conditions, the same dose in the case of a similar specimen, which had been raised artificially to the optimum condition, brought about exaltation of response,-which was found to continue for a fairly long period, after which the effect of the poison was completely overcome (p. 487). The generalisation which has thus been established will be found to be of great significance. A unity of phenomena, as between animal and plant, so fundamental, so detailed, as has been shown to exist, points unmistakably to a basic property of responsiveness common to the two, and mani- fested in both alike by the same effects and modification of effects under stimulation. Nor is the power of response something which makes its appearance suddenly in organic substances only, for it has been demonstrated as existing even in the inorganic. Thus inorganic and organic are held together in a linked continuity. All are responsive, all are depressed by fatigue, all are made excitable by stimulants and rendered irresponsive by ‘ poisons,’ Again, with regard to plants, it may be said that there is hardly a responsive physiological peculiarity in the highest animal that may not be found foreshadowed here. Thus the serial development of the physiological functions in these two cases has been more or less parallel. In the establishment of this generalisation, further, it has been made possible to solve many of the most obscure and difficult problems of Animal Physiology, by studying them under the simple and more manageable conditions of vegetable life. That this is the case has already been seen in many instances, such as that of the polar effects of currents and their reversal under given conditions; in the light shed on the nature of automatism; in the different parts played by external stimulus and internal energy, and their mutual relation ; in the bifurcated expression of incident stimulus as external and internal work; and finally, in the it ——— PHYSIOLOGICAL RESPONSE IN PLANT AND ANIMAL 749 similar action of chemical reagents on plant and animal. It must be remembered, moreover, that one of the greatest advantages to be derived from such a use of the plant as a means of physiolcgical investigation, lies in the fact that this study can be carried on with intact and growing specimens under normal circumstances. The experimental conditions are in this case, therefore, better than those which - correspond to them in regard to animal tissues, since the latter specimen will generally be found to be suffering from the effects of injury, and may therefore have been rendered abnormal to an unknown degree. The study of the re- sponsive phenomena in plants must thus form an integral part of physiological investigation into the various problems relat- ing to the irritability of living tissues, and without such study that investigation must in future be regarded as incomplete. In thus reviewing the movements of plants as a whole, in the light of the investigations which have been described in the course of the present work, we cannot have failed to be struck by the fact that all alike represent, under changing conditions, a single fundamental responsive phenomenon ; -and this response of the plant offers us, as we have seen, a means of tracing the process by which physiological . differentiation from simple to complex has taken place, under the action of stimulus itself. It also enables us to refer a specific differentiation back to definite forces, which have acted asymmetrically upon the organism and induced such a change. We are thus able to see that responsive movements apparently opposite in kind, are nevertheless traceable, not to different specific sensibilities, but to a single universal sensibility, finding different expressions by reason of these _ induced physiological differentiations. Theory of Darwin.—It will be remembered that, accord- ing to the theory of Darwin, a given individual variation which might be in any way advantageous to the organism was perpetuated by the process of natural selection. Thus, in the struggle for existence, only those could continue to live 750 PLANT RESPONSE that were best fitted to their external conditions. Going back a step, however, to the question of the origin of these variations themselves, we find that by some writers they are held to be due to spontaneous unknown causes, in- herent in the organism. Now it would obviously be more satisfactory, since no effect can occur without a cause, if we could assign at least some of these variations to something more definite than this. And Darwin himself was of opinion that variability of every kind was due, directly or indirectly, to changes in the conditions of life. It was difficult, however, to distinguish clearly how much of any given variation was due ‘to the accumulative action of natural selection, and how much to the definite action of the conditions of life.’ ! It was this difficulty which, for example, compelled him to ascribe the movements of plants to specific heliotropic or geotropic sensibilities, acquired as the result of natural selection. The particular reaction or reply to stimulus, manifested by the plant in any special case, was thus to be regarded, not as the direct and necessary result of changes in the environment, but as an adaptive act forced on the species by the struggle for life. Variation as induced by external forces.— With the delicate modes of investigation, however, which are now at the disposal of observers, it has become possible to demon- strate a direct connection between some of the differentia- tions induced in plant-organs and the conditions of the environment. The factors which are conceivable as bringing about variation may be classed under two heads, first, internal or spontaneous, and second, those which arise from external stimulus; but with regard to the first of these we have seen that, as far as experiment has carried us, there is no such thing as spontaneity, in the sense of an effect which occurs without antecedent cause, for the internal energy to which such seeming actions have hitherto been vaguely ascribed has been shown to be itself traceable to external stimulus. And as regards the effect of external stimulus, on ' Darwin, Origin of Species, p. 107. PHYSIOLOGICAL RESPONSE IN PLANT AND ANIMAL 751 the other hand, we have seen that under its action in nature, heterogeneity is evolved out of homogeneity. Thus it is the unequal action of an external force which, for example, causes a radial organ to become anisotropic, with corre- sponding physiological complexity, culminating in dorsi- ventrality. One instance of an organ in which, owing to this differentiation, a movement of apparent advantage to the plant has been induced, is found in the pulvinus of Oxwdzs. Here, by the greater excitability of the lower half, the leaf- lets are made to fold downwards, with the consequence of avoiding too intense illumination, in the responsive move- ment known as diurnal sleep. But the differentiation which we find here is not unique or suddenly evolved, for we find a similar anisotropy even in the pulvinoids of ordinary leaves such as those of Artocarpus. And such physiological differentiation can be traced still further back, to the case of organs which were originally radial. Thus, a long stem, such as that of Cucurbita, happening to become recumbent, becomes also dorsi-ventral, by the unequal action of sunlight on the two sides, the too long excited upper side being now the relatively less excitable. There is again no fixed line of demarcation between this more or less permanent and a transient differentiation ; for when a radial stem of Cucur- bita is acted on by a transient unilateral stimulus, a tempo- rary anisotropy is induced as between the excited and the unexcited sides, the latter, which is fresh, being now the more excitable ; but this anisotropy immediately disappears on the recovery from excitation of the excited side. We may thus have in the same organ at different times a transient anisotropy, lasting for a minute or so, under moderate unilateral stimulation ; a more prolonged aniso- tropy, lasting for an hour or so, under stronger stimulation ; and a permanent differentiation under still stronger and longer continued unilateral stimulation. The difference between the first and the last of these is simply a question of whether the limit of elasticity has been exceeded or not. In a torsioned wire, similarly, on the cessation of moderate 752 PLANT RESPONSE stress, there is complete recovery; and the same _ wire, when torsioned beyond the limits of elasticity, does not recover, but remains permanently strained. In the same _ way, we have seen that the effect of stimulus on a living tissue is to induce a molecular derangement, from which there is complete recovery, with a concomitant recovery of all the physiological properties, if the derangement have been not too great; but, with excessive or long-continued stimulus, the limit of physiological elasticity is exceeded, and a permanent physiological differentiation is thus induced. The same causes, moreover, which initiated the primitive differentiation may now act to induce further a series of com- plex movements. Thus we first see plagiotropic dorsi- ventrality induced in the creeping stem of Cucurbita, by the unilateral action of sunlight ; and then the diurnal periodicity of light and darkness acting on this already differentiated organ to cause a periodic swing, which increases with repeti- tion. In this we have, as has been pointed out, the first stage in the evolution of the nyctitropic movement. Now, it is quite possible that this nyctitropic movement may be found, at least in some cases, to subserve the advantage of the plant; yet it would not be true to say that it was evolved for the purpose of such advantage. Indeed, we have to guard ourselves carefully against being led, by this theory of the final advantage of the plant, into an argument in a circle. Assuming any given movement to be advantageous to the plant, we have first to determine the nature of the mechanism by which it is produced, and secondly to find out what was the exciting cause, and what the character of the conditions under which it first arose. If we are not guided in our inquiry by such considerations, we are liable to be misled in our inferences. One such example was seen in the general belief that a certain specific sensibility resides in the root-tip, by which it is endowed with the faculty of moving away from rough surfaces, which might have been injurious to it. So far from its having any such peculiar faculty, how- ever, we found that when a red-hot wire was presented to it PHYSIOLOGICAL RESPONSE IN PLANT AND ANIMAL 753 the tip moved towards it, and was thus destroyed. The sensitive reaction of the root-tip is thus seen to be, not an adaptive act evolved for the advantage of the plant, but merely an example of the general law, that under moderate stimulation it is the indirect effect of stimulus that reaches the growing organ and causes movement away from, while under strong stimulation the direct effect determines move- ment towards, the source of stimulus. Now, it is perfectly true that had any given reaction been such as, under normal conditions, to bring about the self-destruction of large numbers of organisms, we should not have witnessed the survival of organisms characterised by that particular movement. The plant lives because the physiological differentiations induced in it under natural conditions, and the movements induced under periodic changes of those conditions, are in harmony with the fluctuating forces of the environment. Thus the forced rhythm becomes more deeply impressed with repeti- tion, and the greater is the harmony between this rhythm and the environment, the greater will be its stability under given conditions ; the plant persists, that is to say, because it is perpetually in tune, instead of perpetually at war, with its surroundings. We may take it, therefore, in the case of any particular movement, that it constitutes an expression of this stable relation of the plant to its environment, but not that it represents any deliberate adaptation to such an end. Reverting to the nyctitropic movement in particular, we find it adduced by Darwin as to a certain extent furnishing an example of the influence of heredity on the individual organism, a view which has been questioned. But if we are prepared to give a sufficiently extended and consistent mean- ing to the word, we must accept this, for heredity is essentially the repetition of a past cycle, the persistence of after-effects ; and there are innumerable degrees of such persistence, be- tween that of a transitory after-effect and the phenomenon of absolute persistence, if such occurs. The diurnal periodi- city of Wzmosa, for example, is maintained for several days under unchanging conditions of illumination or of darkness, 256 754 PLANT RESPONSE after which the impressed periodicity is lost. The autum- nal periodicity of trees which grow in temperate climates, where the leaves are habitually shed at the approach of winter, persists for a few or for numerous years when the plant is transferred to a warm climate. Certain species of bacteria, again, can be made to exhibit an artificial change of characteristics which will persist during many generations, even on the return of the organism to normal conditions ; but even after this degree of hereditary persistence of effect has been exhibited, these induced varieties are liable to undergo reversion to their original type. It is thus seen that the persistence of hereditary characteristics is merely relative. Had it been absolute, the species itself would have been immutable. A given rhythm only persists so long as those circumstances which originally occasioned it persist, and during a certain period afterwards ; but this after- or hereditary-effect must, in the non-continuance of the original periodically exciting cause, prove, however long maintained, to be but transitory, like the ultimate arrest of a pendulum when its vibrational energy is exhausted. The phenomenon of life, then, introduces no mystical power, such as would in any way thwart, or place in abeyance, the action of forces already operative. In the machinery of the living, as in that of the non-living, we merely see their transformation, in obedience to the same principle of conservation of energy as obtains elsewhere ; and it may be expected that, in proportion as our power of investigation grows, the origin of each variation of the living organism will be found more and more traceable to the direct or indirect action upon it of external forces, the element of chance being thus progressively eliminated, as the definite sequence of cause and effect comes to be perceived with an increasing clearness ; and only, I venture to think, as this is worked out, can we learn to apprehend fully the true significance of the great Theory of Evolution. Pero sIPiIERD LIST OF EXPERIMENTS DIFFERENTIAL MECHANICAL RESPONSE Mimosa : Response to induction shock . Isometric response Determination of latent nouod and its variation By oa Variation of latent period by fatigue Response to artificial turgidity variation . Effect of excessive turgor on sensibility Erectile response to suction ; Response to transmitted direct Seaton > Cae eee = FU ONO (Coes ON Carat Response to indirect stimulation transversely transmitted Lal by salt solution . : : 12. Response to salt solution in nae tonic panier Biophytum ; Response to indirect stimulation longitudinally Paes Response under favourable tonic condition when chemically stimulated 13. Response to transmitted stimulation with preliminary erectile twitch 14. Response to direct stimulation 15. Effect of load 16. Determination of raitinially effective Shaul ; 17. Effect of cold on latent period 18. Response to increased internal energy 19. Demonstration of factor of internal energy in the process a recovery 20. Response on ‘all or none’ principle . 21. Determination of refractory period 22. Additive effect . Philanthus urinaria : 23. Latent period diminished by intensity of stimulus Ordinary leaves : 24. Response of C2trus decumana 25. Response of Artocarpus 26. Response of Artocarpus to indirect Pienniariont 27. Localisation of motile area in Artocarpus 44, I4I 400 401 273 274 271 756 PLANT RESPONSE Anisotropic organs : PAGE 28. Inorganic response of compound strip : : ; : .) Nee 29. Response of plagiotropic stem of Cucurbita. : : : Os 30. Response of plagiotropic stem of Convolvulus . : ia en 31. Response by collapse of bifurcated 4//cam peduncle < : on 32. Response by uncurling of Passéfora tendril : : : spe SO) 33. Response of spirally cut 4//éwm peduncle by uncurling . : 2) Or 34. Writhing response of spiral tendril of Pass¢flora ; : sf “i Roe LONGITUDINAL MECHANICAL RESPONSE 35- Response of stem of Cuscuta : . : : : : <0 36. Response of root of Bindweed . C : : : : Praha) 37. Response of style of Datura alba . : - 5 : : ny7s 38. Response of coronal filament of Passiflora . : : : St ee 39. Effect of age on excitatory contraction . : : 5 : ooo 40. Effect of season on responsive contraction . : 4 i iri oe ELECTRIC RESPONSE 41. Response to direct stimulation . . 2 : : ; eae 42. Response to transmitted direct stimulation . : : é ci i 43. Indirect hydrostatic and true excitatory response. : : ea; 44. Electric test of differential excitability : : : : -* Jae 45. Response to moderate stimulation of tip of growing organ : = ai 46. Response to strong stimulation of tip 4 : : : 517, 518 47. Response to direct stimulation of growing region. : : . 518 48. Response to moderate transverse stimulation. : : : ee 49. Response to strong transverse stimulation “ : A * sBrON 522 ELECTROTACTILE RESPONSE 50. Electrotactile response in AZmosa stem. é : : Ae Abo DEATH-RESPONSE 51. Determination of death-point by electrical method 3 ‘ = dG 52. Determination of death-point by spasmodic lateral response Gy meee 53- Determination of death-point by spasmodic movement of uncurling . 155 54. Determination of death-point by sudden volumetric contraction - 156 55- Determination of death-point from critical point of thermo-mechanical inversion . ; , 5 ; : : 5 : . 168 56. Constancy of critical whe of inversion : : es | 57. Modification of thermo-mechanical curve ehowiee affect of age. | Lo 58. Thermo- mechanical curve of A//mosa . Ramen 59. Duplication of rigor-point in J/inosa by shame Beene : -, £79 60. Translocation of death-point by unfavourable weather , . 172, L760 61. Translocation of death-point by poisonous solution . : : - saLOO 62. Translocation of death-point by fatigue —, ; : ; - +, ee CLASSIFIED LIST OF EXPERIMENTS 757 63. Death-response of flowers : : : : - - : 64. Thermograph of regional death . : ; : : : Loos MULTIPLE AND AUTONOMOUS RESPONSE 65. Multiple electromotive response 66. Multiple electrotactile response 67. Multiple mechanical response in Biophytum : 68. Cyclic variation of multiple response in Azophytem 69. Cyclic variation of multiple visual response 70. Multiple response in Desmodium originally at medal 71. Multiple response to constant chemical stimulus in Azophyturm 72. Multiple response to constant current in Averrhoa 73- Multiple response to constant light in retina 74. Multiple response to constant light in Bzophytem 75. Multiple response to constant light in Desmodzzm : 76. Initiation of autonomous response in Bore above fhemio- tonic minimum - ¢ : 77. Resumption of autonomous response in Desaaeans By accession Ae energy : : : : 78. Resumption of autonomous ee in Dadi andes cite internal hydrostatic pressure . , 79. Localisation of seat of autonomous excitation in Dechodean 80. ‘Systolic’ and ‘diastolic’ positions in Desmodium pulsation 81. Incapability of tetanus in Desmodium $2. Measurement of period and amplitude of Desenar Seetiie fon photographic record : 83. Measurement of period and amplitude of Deguaean pulaiod fou spark record : $4. Periodic variation in een en pneanon A : ae See also Periodic variation of growth and Gasieiedi response 472, 473> SUCTIONAL RESPONSE 85. Record of suction, Unbalanced Method . 86. Shoshungraphic record under Hydrostatic Balance 87. Shoshungraphic record under Hydraulic Balance 88. Renewal of suctional response under stimulus 89. Periodic variation of transpiration in Czczurbzta See also Mechanical, Growth, and Torsional riveted 3 to detional activity . : . 5 - ; 5 . - 400, 426, GROWTH-RESPONSE 90. Balanced Method of Crescographic record gt. Multiple growth-pulsation : ¢ : 92. Cyclic groupings of multiple growth- pletion - : ae e4 Lis 93. Resumption of growth by stimulus : : 94. Resumption of growth in drought-rigored Cucustita afte supply of water 327 470 474 412 417 425 425 426 758 95- 96. 97- 08. 99. 100. IOI. 102. 103. 104. 105. 106. 107. 108. 109. 110. Lee 112. ie 114. 115. 116. P77: 118. 119. 120. I2I. 122. 13. 124. 125. 126. 127, PLANT RESPONSE PAGE Growth in response to hydraulic transmission of energy... si). ae Effect of decreased suctional activity on growth E ; : “AZo Effect of increased hydrostatic pressure on growth of Balsam os oe des Effect of increased hydrostatic pressure on growth of Crinum Lily . 428 Effect of tension on growth : ; : : = - = 4g2 Bifurcated expression of stimulus in conereate response and resump- tion of growth . é : : : : : ; : ARE Similarity of response in stationary and growing organs ; crea. 12 (6 Retardation of growth by external stimulus. : 3 : - 436 Detection of latent stimulus by negative after-effect . 3: Bie deg 3: 13 Constancy of sum of direct effect and negative after-effect - . 460 Vanishing of latent component above the emo : : eon Variation of receptivity with temperature : ¢ 460 Direct and indirect effect of stimulus on plant in ditecen tonic conditions .° A : : 462, 463 Diurnal variation of rate of ¢ ‘row in One Sativa . ; : = A Diurnal variation of rate of growth in seedling of Zamarindus indica 474 Effect of high frequency Tesla current on growth . : ; . 619 Effect of Hertzian waves on growth . : : : : tee OnS RESPONSIVE GROWTH-CURVATURES Response of pistil of JZsa to unilateral thermal stimulation. ~ es Response of Crocus to moderate unilateral stimulation of tip ae en RPC, Response of Crocus to stronger stimulation. ‘ - ; « 4526 Response of Croczs to direct stimulation of growing region . = wagon Response of root of Bindweed to unilateral stimulus of tip 5 7520 Response of root of Bindweed to stronger stimulation of tip ee, Response of root of Bindweed to unilateral stimulation of growing region : ; ; : 5 . ; : ; : oh OT TORSIONAL RESPONSE Torsional response of pulvinus of J/¢osa under lateral stimulus of light. 5 . s O56 Torsional response of Saigare of Winata cnaee iver ‘heres stimulus . ; : : : : : : : « O59 Torsional response of inorganic sheen strip . 3 ~ on 000 Modification of torsional response in AZmosa by reversal of differential excitability of pulvinus : é : : ; : . 661 Modification of torsional response in J/imosa by increase of differential excitability of pulvinus . ; < tos ee Torsional response in Z7ythrina indica anaet ee geotanit stimulus . ; ? . 664 Modification of eenteanne torsion by Bh of digeteuteal exttaes bility. : : ; : =. snOus Modification of geotropic torsion by increase ‘af differential excitabanie 665 Autonomous torsion of stems . ‘ ‘ ; ; : 4 . 668 CLASSIFIED LIST OF EXPERIMENTS 759 PAGE 128. Enhancement of rate of autonomous torsion by increased suctional activity . : 669 129. Modification of rate of autonomous torsion ate slectrieal current . . 670 UNIFORM RESPONSE, FATIGUE, AND STAIRCASE EFFECT 130. Uniform electrical response in Radish. : : . 104 See also Uniform mechanical responses in Bebiiim amd Datura . 24 131. Staircase effect in mechanical response of Zucharis . S85 tig 132. Staircase effect, followed by fatigue, in the response of eas Ase tape y 133. Staircase effect, followed by fatigue, in the response of Bryophyl/um 122 134. Staircase effect in inorganic response of Galena . : : 5 6. PME 135. Fatigue in mechanical response of Datura. : ; alos 136. Fatigue-reversal under continuous stimulation in Passifare oS 137. Fatigue-reversal under continuous stimulation in J/mosa . 109, 113, 552 138. Periodic fatigue in Uricls . : . . . é : . 108 139. Fatigue in electrical response of Radish _. < LOS 140. Fatigue-reversal under continuous stimulation in gece response of Celery . < ; : : : = ElO7, 141. Alternate fatigue 1 in electrical and Recharical response : Pr elOO 142. Fatigue in inorganic response: arsenic . : ‘ : 5 ENG, 143. Fatigue in inorganic response : india-rubber : : A Ce oO) 144. Alternating fatigue in arsenic . : : Ss 145. Apparent fatigue in response of plant, ite to iehieecaon of energy . 127 ADDITIVE EFFECT AND RELATION BETWEEN STIMULUS AND RESPONSE 146. Additive effect in mechanical response. : ‘ : : 54 §o 147. Additive effect in electrical response . , : ° : sles 148. Genesis of tetanus in mechanical response ‘ 99 149. Relation between intensity of stimulus and See ahe of accbaateal response : : 96 150. Relation between ice of sdovalus al afephende of aleetical response . : - OF 151. Influence of tonic Beaders on Poaticn bereed peaies and respon- sive growth-retardation . ; : : : : : oe 407, POLAR EFFECTS OF CURRENTS Pulvinar response : 152. Action of feeble E.M.F. on JZzosa, Mono-polar Method . LO? 153. Action of feeble E.M.F. on AZimosa, Bi-polar Method. Alo 154. Action of feeble E.M.F. on 4zophytwm, Mono-polar Method alo 155. Action of feeble E.M.F. on 4zophytum, Bi-polar Method . Lod! 156. Effect of moderate E.M.F. on Bzophytum . : : 5 YS 157. Effect of moderate E.M.F. on highly excitable ane : pee lO”, 158. Effect of moderate E.M.F. on Desmodium . ; . : Sy Boe 760 PLANT RESPONSE PAGE Reversing effect of high E.M.F. : 159. Astage, Bzophytum . : : - : . é Pete 215.1 160. Mimosa. : : : : : : : = n2Ox! 161. S#stage, Averrhoa : : : . : : : 4 eee 162. and fatigue, J/imosa : - 208 163. Localised effect of Anode and KRathode on aalte inus ne i jibe wnaica 2 ; : : : 3 - - «le S55 Death-response : 164. Polar effects of moderate E.M.F. on death-response__.. : «ES 165. Polar effects of high E.M.F. on death-response : 6 eee, Glow-response : 166. Polar action of moderate E.M.F. on glow-response of firefly . ete 167. Reversal of effects by high E.M.F. or by tissue-modification . . 213 Growth-response: 168. Anodic and kathodic effects on growth of root of Bindweed . SS 169. Galvanotropic response of Crinum Lily . : ; : Pree ijs(e) 170. Indirect effect of polar excitation on Champaca . : : - 559 171. Effect of electrification of soil on growth of Oryza sativa . fi 5,500 See also Galvanotaxis . : : ; : : 2 : . 698 EXCITABILITY AND CONDUCTIVITY 172. Diminution or abolition of motile excitability by anesthetics . . 220 173. Diminution or abolition of motile excitability by cold’. 4 -— 220 174. Diminution or abolition of motile excitability by fatigue . - oueen 175. Diminution or abolition of motile excitability by An-electrotonus . 236 176. Increase of excitability by Kat-electrotonus “ : “ Spon 2s 177. ‘Developing’ action of Kathode . : : : =e02Bo 178. Diminution or abolition of conductivity by anesthe : ph ees 179. Diminution or abolition of conductivity by cold. : : vance 180. Diminution or abolition of conductivity by fatigue. ; parr, Lee 181. Diminution or abolition of conductivity by anodic block : <5239 182. An-electrotonic and Kat-electrotonic action on conductivity ace ete 183. Depression of receptive excitability by anzesthetics : : es 2 184. Receptivity verses motile excitability : : 2 : os Lot 185. Excitability verses conductivity . ; ‘ : : . 227220 VELOCITY OF TRANSMISSION OF EXCITATORY WAVE 186. Detection of excitatory pulse during transit by Chemical Method. . 255 187. Detection of excitatory pulse during transit by Electrotactile ; Method . ; : 257 188. Detection of excitatory piles anne transit = Blectniiene Method . : : 261 189. Determination of welaies oF tegemieson in Bioghiina te mechanical response ; : : : ‘ ; : . 241 190. IgI. 192. 193. 194. 195. CLASSIFIED LIST OF EXPERIMENTS Determination of centrifugal and centripetal velocities Effect of fatigue on velocity . Effect of intensity of stimulus on velocity . Effect of lowering of temperature on velocity . Effect of rise of temperature on velocity : : Relative velocities in various plants, ‘ sensitive’ and ordinary . EFFECT OF TEMPERATURE ON RESPONSE Mechanical response : 196. 197. 198. Effect of cold on longitudinal contractile response : a Effect of rise of temperature on longitudinal contractile response. Effect of cyclic rise and fall of temperature Electric response : 199. 200. 201. 202. Diminution of response in Zucharis by lowering of temperature After-effect of cold on Ivy, Holly, Zucharis Effect of rise of temperature . - - Effect of cyclic variation of temperature Autonomous pulsation of Desmodium : 203. 204. 205. 206. 207. 208. 209. Effect of slight cooling on amplitude and period Effect of cooling to thermo-tonic minimum on amplitude and neuied Effect of raising from thermo-tonic minimum upwards Effect of continuous rise of temperature on amplitude and period Determination of temperature minimum Determination of temperature maximum Persistence of rhythmic activity at maximum. Suctional response : 210. 211. 2T2: DER. 214. 215. 216. Determination of effect of cold by Unbalanced Method Determination of effect of cold by Hydrostatic Balance . Determination of effect of cold by Hydraulic Balance Effect of warm water, Unbalanced Method Effect of warm water, Hydrostatic Balance Effect of warm water, Hydraulic Balance Persistence of suction when root killed by hot water . Growth : 217, 218. 219. 220. 221. Determination of various rates at different temperatures by Discon- tinuous observations Determination of various rates at Gimeeehs annul hee fon Them: crescent curve : - Determination of various rates at different Gonennn: Method of Balance - Determination of various rates at aera esas ey Method of Excitatory Response . Determination of optimum point by Method of Balece 761 PAGE 243 245 246 248 249 252 143 144 146 140 I4I 143 145 332 335 336 334 331 331 431 373 376 379 375 378 380 381 442 447 450 452 451 762 222. ae. PLANT RESPONSE Translocation of optimum point Persistence of rhythmic activity at maximum . Autonomous torsion : 224. Effect of temperature on rate of autonomous torsion . EFFECT OF CHEMICAL AGENTS Mechanical response : 225. 226. 227. 228. 229. 230. 231. . Effect of chlorine gas 232 Effect of carbonic acid . Effect of hydrogen . Effect of carbon iecaebide 3 Effect of alcohol : Effect of ether on longitudinal ea pene Effect of ether on motile response of A//mosa Effect of acid Autonomous pulsation of Desmodium : 233. 234. 235. 236. 237- 238. 239. 240. Effect of ether Effect of alcohol Effect of carbonic acid gas Effect of copper sulphate solution Effect of barium salt solution Arrest at ‘diastole’ by acid Arrest at ‘systole’ by alkali . Antagonistic actions of acid and alkali Suctional response : 241. 242. Action of KNO, solution ; Osmotic versus excitatory action of NaCl stiaee Growth: 243. 244. 245. 246. 247. 248. 249. 250. 2s 252. 253. 254. 255. Effect of carbonic acid . Action of ether, external application . Action of ether, internal application Excitatory effect of dilute solution of sodium capone Effect of solution of sugar Effect of alcohol , Effect of acid inducing olawation aa arrest . Effect of alkali inducing contraction and arrest . Antagonistic actions of alkali and acid . ; Action of strong sodium chloride solution under difeneat theca tonic conditions . : : Differing effects of poisons on difecent : consafations? Opposite effects of same poisonous dose on different ‘ constintf anes Opposite effects of large and small doses . PAGE 454 432 668 ri 132 CLASSIFIED LIST OF EXPERIMENTS 763 Growth-curvature : PAGE 256. Chemo-tropic effect of alkali. é : : : : AO 257. Antagonistic chemo-tropic effect of acid : : : : . 548 258. Effect of copper sulphate solution. : ae : See aeyels) 259. Effect of sugar solution : : : , ; : : . 549 See also Chemo-tactic response : : : : : ue OOS GEOTROPISM 260. Apo-geotropic response of scape of Uriclis . 499 261. Response of Crzz Lily under unilateral Bene of petiieles Ben, viele 262. Apo-geotropic response at inclinations of 45° and 135° . : . 501 263. Demonstration of apo-geotropic curvature as induced by responsive contraction . - - 504 264. Effect of alternate poaiecions a cota on upper ata cen apres on apo-geotropic response : 505 265. Darwinian curvature and its reversal Grider different intensities a stimulation . F = ee 1530 266. Determination of effect af TepataGon of ae tip on exeitatiity 5 bya 267. Effect of gravitational stimulus on autonomous torsion . ; eu O7T See also Response to stimulation of tips of root and shoot . ee 520 Also Differential geotropic sensibility of dorsi-ventral organs . 662, 666 EFFECT OF VISIBLE AND INVISIBLE RADIATIONS 268. Response of A/¢mosa to electric radiation . - ; . 619 269. Discrimination of effects of thermal radiation and of Prexaeue a Ons 270. Response of A/zsa to unilateral thermal radiation . : Ou 271. Response of Zamarinduzs to continuous unilateral thermal dite 617 272. Effect of diffuse stimulation of light on growth of sub-tonic tissue . 574 273. Effect of diffuse stimulation of light on stationary radial organ = Gif 274. Retarding effect of light on longitudinal growth : : oaue, 572 275. Oscillatory growth-response under continuous action of light . ee S74 276. Effect of light on autonomous torsion 5 3 2 : ee OGO HELIOTROPISM In radial growing organs: 277. Positive responses of Sz7agzs to moderate light : : » 593, 609 278. Positive responses of cooled tendril of Passz/fora ° : 5 eee OFT 279. Positive responses of tendril of V¢zs to feeble light ; : oe OU 280. Neutral response of Szafzs under strong stimulation . : = OOO 281. Neutral response of tendril of Passz/#ora to strong stimulation = OUT 282. Negative response of root of Szzapzs : : : ) A) BODO 283. Negative response of shoot of Cvocus on stimulation of (aye) = oc 2 OO 284. Negative response of tendril of V7¢/s under strong stimulation . . 611 In plagiotropic growing organs: 285. Negative response of stem of Cucurbita to strong vertical light . 626 286. Negative response of stem of /Aomea to strong vertical light . G25 207. 208. 299. 300. 301. 302. PLANT RESPONSE . Negative response of stem of JZ/mosa to strong vertical light . Positive response of terminal leaflet of Desmodium, stimulated alter- nately from above and below . ; ; ; é : ee . Orientating action of light . Positive response of Erythrina indica (aigeo jon wiaveon am . Positive response of Rodzz7a (diurnal sleep movement) . é . Positive response of Cltorza ternatea (diurnal sleep movement) . . Neutral response of Desmodium leaflet under long-continued stimu- lation : . . Positive response % Mitaasas ee siiuleted alternate from hoes and below, by light of feeble intensity . . Transition of response in J/zmosa, from positive, caene ge to negative, under continuous light from above . Negative response of leaflet of ABzophytum (diurnal slecaie move- ment) . 5 Negative response of leaflet of Onan (qaeaat cece: Multiple heliotropic response . : : : - 13035 a 60%, Multiple merging into continuous heliotropic response Localisation of heliotropic sensibility in pulvinus . - . "588, Lamina not perceptive organ to stimulus of light Absence of specific dia-heliotropic tendency in leaves In ordinary leaves: 303. 304. 305. 306. Positive response of leaf of MWangifera indica Negative response of Artocarpus Torsional response to lateral stimulation at cone leaflet ae Desmodium . : See also Zorstonal pesca of drs: vent) ai organs . Limits of flexibility in various leaves Nyctitropic movement : 307- 308. 309. 310. Ee Diurnal movement of plagiotropic stem. Diurnal movement of leaf of Azophytim Nyctitropic movement of J/zmosa leaf, due to camila naomi action : Nyctitropic movement ss primary ipeliole of eo. ; By el: Impressed heliotropic periodicity in Sunflower Heliotropic action on autonomous response : bee 313: 314. 315: Initiation of autonomous response by light in Desmodium . Changes of amplitude in autonomous pulsation of Desmodium Periodic reversals of relative rapidities of up and down beats in Desmodium . : Modification of autonomous torsion See also Phototaxis . PAGE 625 587 587 629 629 629 653 661 647 680 EP Ne Xx ACELLULAR organ, responsive curvature of, 506 Acid, chemo-tropic action of, 548 ,, effect on mechanical response, 136 Acid and alkali, antagonistic action of, on cardiac pulsation, 351 a z= 5 ne on chemo-tropic response, 548 na - an 33 on Desmodium pulsation, 352 “ ae 55 FA on growth, 484 Additive effect, 95 After-effect, detection of absorbed stimulus by negative, 459 0 factors modifying, 468 . persistence of, 310 a positive and negative, 457 AC “e 7 constancy of sum of, 460 Age, effect of, on contractile response, 79 = », on thermo-mechanical inversion, 171 Alcohol, effect of, on Desmodium pulsation, 325 és Fe on growth, 482 : Ss on mechanical response, 133 All or none’ principle in response of Brophytim, 272 3 a Pe cardiac tissue, 353 ‘Allium, death-response of, 157 », mechanical response of, 87 Anesthetics, action of, on autonomous response, 324 * e on conductivity, 220, 229 Bs $3 on excitability, 220 - - on mechanical response, 134, 135 se ‘5 on receptivity, 224 An-electrotonus, 232 Anisotropy, induction of, by cold, 82 ” 5 by fatigue, 83 5 = by light, $4 a necessary condition for lateral response, 44 Annual rings, 474 Anode, effect of, on growth, 556 Anodic block, 233 Arisema, curious response of, 533 Arsenic, fatigue-reversal in, 119, 575, 653 766 PLANT RESPONSE Artocarpus, effect of over-turgidity, 58 ‘ a localisation of motile areas in, 58 s response of, 55, 420, 653 } Ascent of sap, mechanics of, 390 <5 rapidity of, 392 5 theories of, 359 a uni-directioned flow of, 390 ; Autonomous excitation, seat of, in Desmodium, 297 3 response, continuity with multiple, 290, 291 $5 $5 induction of, by hydrostatic pressure in cardiac tissue, 307 as ih AB 35 ne in Desmodium, 307 e 2 rn by accession of energy in Biophytum, 305 a Be He nf >» in Desmodium, 306 5 + periodic groupings of, 291, 350 Avena sativa, heliotropic response of, 604 Balsam, effect of hydrostatic pressure on growth of, 428 », growth-pulsation of, 432 Barium chloride, effect on Desmodium pulsation, 351 Bindweed, effect of section of root-tip on excitability of, 541 “3 polar effect of current in the growth of root of, 556 33 response of root of, to thermal stimulus, 76, 526, 527 Biophytum, additive effect in, 274 5 autonomous response of, 305 % diurnal sleep movement of, 634 * effect of anzesthetics, 223, 225 sn 55 an- and kat-electrotonus, 232-236 a a cold on excitability of, 222, 224 is ats »» latent period of, 141 3 55 load on response of, 25 3 electric response of, 34, 37 mi erectile response of, 24, 37, 400 *F minimally effective stimulus in, 27 f multiple response of, 282 e¢ seg. =f normal mechanical response of, 24, 37 ee nyctitropic movement of, 680 ‘3 polar effect of current on, 195, 197 Ae refractory period of, 273 HS response on ‘all or none’ principle, 272 Bi-polar method, 191 Botrydium granulatum, phototaxis of, 497 Brown, H., on diffusion through multi-perforate septum, 399 Bryophyllum calcynum, response of, 62 CARBON disulphide, effect of, on mechanical response, 132 Carbonic acid, effect of, on Desmodium pulsation, 325 =F - a on growth, 480 9 re 5 on mechanical response, 131 Chemical method, determination of velocity by, 255 INDEX Chemical reagents, effect on autonomous response, 323-326 3° Chemo-taxis, oe) ” growth, 479-488 oe », mechanical response, 131-137 a », suctional response, 383, 384, 385 an modification of effect of, 322 698 Chemo-tropic response: effect of acid, 548 ” alkali, 548 ry, .s copper sulphate soJution, 548 of 50 sugar solution, 549 99 Le) Chlorine, effect of, on mechanical response, 137 Ciesielski on effect of amputation of root-tip, 539 Citrus decumana, response of leaf of, 64 Clitoria ternatea, diurnal sleep movement of, 629 Cold, abolition of autonomous response by, 141, 331 Led bed > 29 ”) 92 mechanical response by, 141 suctional response by, 374 diminution of conductivity by, 221, 249 prolongation of latent period by, 141, 264 Cold-rigor, 171 Colocasia, excretion of water in, 397 Conduction, channels of, 60, 250 39 > of excitation and of sap, 391 pseudo-, 251 Conductivity, difference between longitudinal and transverse, 250 a2 29 preferential direction of, 242 seasonal variation of, 602 variation of, by aneesthetics, 223 a by cold, 221, 249 3 by electrotonus, 233 a by fatigue, 222 - by rise of temperature, 222, 249 Conductivity-variation, response by, 39 ‘Constitution,’ elements determining, 478 29 modifying effect of, on action of drugs, 487 Convolvulus, death-response of flower of, 182, 183 ” 99 response of plagiotropic stem to light, 625 “3 », to thermal shocks, 86 9 Coprinus, action of light on, 569 Counterpoise, importance of, 19 Crescograph, 412-416 Crinum Lily, apo-geotropic response of, 498, 500, 504 9 be) 29 direct and indirect after-effect of stimulus in, 460 effect of increased hydrostatic pressure on growth of, 428 ss tension on growth of, 433 », unilateral pressure of particles on, 497 excitatory response at different temperatures of, 453 optimum point of growth in, 448, 451 thermo-crescent curve of growth in, 447 767 768 PLANT RESPONSE Crocus, growth-curvature by stimulation of tip and growing region of shoot, 526, 527 Cucurbita, diurnal variation of rate of transpiration in, 472 =e initiation of growth-pulsation in, 426 = response of plagiotropic stem of, to thermal shocks, 86 3 ” ” ” to light, 626 Cuscuta, response of stem of, 77 Cyneree, response of filaments of, 69 Czapek on localisation of gravi-perception at root-tip, 542 s, on radial pressure theory, 494 DARKNESS, modification of sensitiveness in, 22, 50 Darwin, Francis, on effect of alternating stimulation, 685 = >, On growth-curvature under light and gravitation, 494 Darwin on localised geotropic sensibility at root-tip, 539 », OM sensitiveness of radicle, 513 Darwinian curvature, 536 » theory, 749 Datura alba, style of : death-discoloration of, 183 or > iy death-response of, 160 a 53 a effect of light on growth, 571 3 5 . lowering of death-point by fatigue, 185 = a 5 response of growing and stationary, 434, 435, 436 os sp a response to thermal stimulus, 78 » > ” tetanus in, 99 Death-discoloration, 183 Death-point, constancy of, 173 op determination of, by electrical response, 149 a = by movement of uncurling, 165 < i by spasmodic lateral response, 150 or ¥ by thermo-mechanical inversion, 161 s ag by volumetric contraction, 156 i electrotonic effect on, 185, 186, 199, 207 translocation of, by age, 170 < a. by fatigue, 185 a 5 by poison, 180 ¥ 5 by unfavourable weather, 172 Death-response, effect of chemical agents on, 179, 180 5 in flowers, 181 5 physiological nature of, 152 Death-spasm in plants, 150, 155, 156 De Candolle, theory of heliotropism, 580 Desmodium gyrans, vaultiple response of, 293 * 5, polar excitation of, 292 ms », seat of autonomous excitation in, 297 =“ »» Significance of up and down movement in, 330 Desmodium pulsation, diurnal record of, 470 x a incapability of tetanus, 346 - is effect of anzesthetics on, 323 INDEX Desmodium pulsation, effect of acid on, 352 » » 3 alcohol on, 324 » 50 ss alkali on, 352 »” rr) 36 barium salt on, 351 ” “ =. carbonic acid on, 325 » 2 » copper sulphate solution on, 326 » 3 9H light on, 691, 692 ” 29 » temperature on, 332-33 a 34 period and amplitude of, 317 a 3 periodic variation of, 341 »” a similarity between Lzophyfum pulsation and, 345 »” >» 5 “Kc cardiac pulsation and, 345-354 » spark-record of, 327 A 30 temperature, maximum of, 331 » >» thermo-tonic minimum of, 331 Detmer on photonasty, 643 Development, thermographic, 184 De Vries on epi- and hypo-nasty, 643 Ag on heliotropic action on leaves, 622 ~ on plasmolytic method, 549 Dia-geotropism, so-called, 640, 654 Dia-heliotropism, so-called, 640-654 Differential response in plant, 14 af »» Magnification of, 51 Ss Pe of compound strip, 13 Directive versus non-directive action of light, 634 Diurnal sleep, 629, 633, 634 Dorsi-ventral organ, differential geotropic action inducing torsion in, 662 Er eS 5% heliotropic action inducing torsion in, 658 3 »» no specific sensibility in, 577 Dutrochet on conducting elements in J7Z/¢70sa, 251 ELECTRIC radiation, effect of, on AZ¢mosa, 619 a response, difference of excitability detected by, 38 a 55 laws of, 42 3 5 multiple, 279 a A of inorganic substance, 40 Pe ne simultaneous mechanical and, 34 a3 a to direct stimulation, 32, 518 “ 40 to indirect stimulation, 37, 517 a ie true excitatory negative and hydrostatic positive, 35, 37 ‘A waves, effect of, on growth, 618 ‘Electrification ’ of soil, effect of, on growth, 560 Electromotive Method, detection of excitatory wave by, 261 Electrotactile Method of detection of excitation, 256, 280 Electrothermic stimulator, 17 Electrotonus, variation of excitability by, 234, 235, 236 Elfving on geotropic action on grass-haulm, 496 Erythrina indica, differential geotropic response of, 664, 665 3D 770 PLANT RESPONSE Erythrina indica, heliotropic response of, 629 a »» polar effect of current on, 555 Eucharis Lily, cold-rigor in, 141, 172 Excitability, seasonal variation of, 80, 474 variation of, by aneesthetics, 220 . x a by cold, 220 “5 ar by electrotonus, 234 “8 2 by fatigue, 221 + versus conductivity, 227 3 »» receptivity, 226 Excitation, longitudinal conduction of, 516, 517, 526, 532, 604 = mechanical versus protoplasmic theory of, 189, 229 = transverse conduction of, 519, 528, 529, 532 Excitatory contraction, physiological modification of, 79 o7 discharge, preferential direction of, 243 35 versus hydrostatic effect, 35, 47 ey wave, centrifugal velocity of, 243 oe », centripetal velocity of, 243 fe ;, determination of velocity of, by Chemical Method, 255 he. 99 ” 3 by Electromotive Method, 260 ” > » 45 by Electrotactile Method, 25 » 2 99 “5 by Mechanical Method, 240 rs »» modification of velocity of, by cold, 248 » » ” A: by fatigue, 245 os 93 99 a by intensity of stimulus, 246 9 29 ss + by rise of temperature, 249 5 », velocity of, in plant and animal, 252 FATIGUE, alternating, 106 os effect of, on conductivity and excitability, 111 - sis on electrical response, 105 An 5 on mechanical response, 105 = Pe on velocity of transmission, 244 Lf periodic, 65, 108 Ae reversal of normal polar effect by, 207 Fatigue-reversal under continuous stimulation, in arsenic, 119 ae 5 e i in india-rubber, 120 oy i ‘ af in plant, 63, 107, 108, 109 Fick on velocity of nervous impulse, 246 Firefly, glow-response of, 211 Flexibility, characteristic limits of, 646 Flowers, death-response of, 181 Food-substance, translocation of, 398 Forced vibration, 684 Frank on heliotropic response of A/archantia, 622 », on transverse heliotropism and geotropism, 641 Frost, effect on leaves, 400 GALENA, staircase response in, 12I Galvanotaxis, 698 INDEX Galvanotropic response, 558 Geotropic response, at inclinations of 45° and 135°, 500 99 29 ” 9° 2? effect of unilateral application of cold on, 504, 505 mechanics of, 495 method of recording, 499 of shoot and root, 544 torsional, 664 true character of, 502 sensibility, localisation at root-tip, 539 Geotropism, differential, 663 radial pressure and statolithic theories of, 494 Glow-response, 211 Grass-haulms, geotropic response of, 496, 508 growth of, on klinostat, 509 Greely, A. W., galvanotaxis of favamecia, 698 Growing organ and pulvinus, identical nature of responses in, 534 a9 Growth, anodic and kathodic effect on, 556 antagonistic action of acid and alkali on, 484 apparent arrest of, at high temperature, 432 balanced record of, 413 R daily periodicity in, 473, 474 direct and indirect stimulus on, 438 effect of acid on, 483 be) 99 ” Growth-curvature, induction of, by electrical current, 553 2 Aes 39 le} alcohol on, 482 alkali on, 483 carbonic acid on, 479 electric waves on, 618 ‘ electrification’ of soil on, 560 ether on, 480 NaCl solution on, 485 Na,Co, solution on, 482 poisonous solution on, 486 sugar solution on, 482 external stimulus on, 434, 435, 436 laws of, 439 opposite effects on, of chemical agents on different ‘ constitutions,’ 487 2) optimum temperature of, 448, 451 ? b er of large and small doses, 488 translocation of, 454 periodic variation of rate of, 470, 473, 474 polar action of current on, 557, 558, 559 relation of, to hydrostatic pressure, 428 ” ) ”) to tension, 432 ms by unilateral stimulus, 525, 527, 528, 529 Growth-pulsation, multiple, 417, 432 periodic groupings of, 417, 425 Growth, renewal of, by external stimulus, 425 99 2» by positive turgidity-variation, 426 771 772 PLANT RESPONSE Growth-response and excitatory response, 418 ae similayities between pulvinar and, 437 HABERLANDT on transmission of excitation in J/¢osa, 251 Re on statolithie theory, 494 Heart-beat, theories of, 347 Hedera helix, seasonal variation of heliotropic response in, 623 Heliotropic excitation, transmission of, 604 5 negative response of dorsi-ventral organs: leaf of Artocarpus, 653 -. Me 3 x Ls », AWimosa, 631, 632 - a we os Fr leaflet of Brophyltum, 634 ce) +) ” 29 ” ” Oxalis, 633 Hd ts AS 5 A plagiotropic stem of Carcz7- bita, 626 A - As sc 3 plagiotropic stem of Lpomea, 625 . As eS Pe A plagiotropic stem of Mimosa, 625 Ae 3 Fy radial organs : hypocotyl of Szapzs, 609 ‘ 5 ne 5 == Lepidium, 608 - A Ey 53 P 3 root of Szvapzs, 601 o5 sa - =. =p shoot of Crocs, 601 aa ~ 33 ee 25 tendril of Vzt7s, 611 as neutral response: leaflet of Desmodium, 604 Ss ap —5 Lepidium, 608 ks > 53 Stnapis, 609 a3 e5 3% tendril of Pass¢fora, 611 a perception, absence of, in lamina, 588, 650, 651 ae positive response of dorsi-ventral organs : leaf of Mangzi/era indica, 653 re ae fT; 55 », leaflet of Cltoria ternatea, — 639 3 ae ir 3 s, leaflet of Desmodium, 586, 587, 628 ae s aA Ar 5, leaflet of Arxvthrina, 629 = 3 Ae AP », leaflet of Robznza, 629 y9 i 5 radial organs: Lepidium, 608 ” ” ” ” ” Stnapis, 593, 609 ss a ¥ Py Fr tendril of Passtflora, 611 ” ” ” ” ” ” Vitis, 611 a Recorder, 588-591 =p response, definition of positive and negative, 581 AP An intensity of stimulus on, 608, 609 4 Es modifying action of anisotropy and conductivity on, 608, 613, 628 is i multiple, 303, 584, 604, 634 9 . a merging into continuous, 586 7 if positive, negative, and dia-, 609 ae re seasonal variation of, 602, 623 ” if torsional, 659, 661 INDEX Heliotropic sensitiveness, localisation of, 604, 650 99 Ap supposed absence of, in organs, 610 Heliotropism, theory of Darwin, 582 ”” de Candolle, 580 9 Hydraulic model, 31 ae response, 363 Hydrogen, effect of, on mechanical response, 131 Hydrostatic disturbance, effect on electric response, 35 9 9 - »» Mechanical response, 24 versus true excitatory effect, 35, 47 INDIA-RUBBER, contractile response of, 12 LP fatigue-reversal of, 120 Indicator diagram, 2 Inner stimuli cause of growth, 421 rh) 39 derived from external, 355 Inorganic response, 12, 13, 40, 118 99 ” 9 abolition of, by poison, 40 fatigue in, 119, 120 staircase effect in, 121 Internal energy. 308 99 » LB) 9 29 antagonistic action of, and external stimulus, 403 effect of, on amplitude of pulsation, 338 », on frequency, 338 a factor of recovery, 401 spontaneous movement initiated by excess of, 305, 306 tonic condition dependent on, 308, 314 Tpomaa, torsional response of, 670 Isometric record, 25 Isotonic record, 25 Ivy, seasonal variation of heliotropic sensibility of, 623 », Variation of electrical response by temperature, 141 JENNINGS on chemotaxis of protozoa, 699 KAT-ELECTROTONUS, 235 Kathode, effect of, on growth, 556 ae excitatory effect of, 192-197 Kuhne on polar excitation of protozoa, 200 Kunchangraph, 71 LAMINA not perceptive organ, 60, 588, 650, 651 Latent period, diminution of, by strong stimulus, 271 33 be) ” 99 99 29 in AZimosa, 22, 268 prolongation of, by cold, 141, 268 29 by fatigue, 269 Lateral geotropism, 672 99 be) be) response, general absence of, in radial organs, 50 necessary conditions for, 44, 48 774 PLANT RESPONSE Laws of electrical response, 42 growth, 439 growth-curvature, 535 mechanical response, 51 polar action of currents, 199, 207, 558 torsional response, 662 eat es, dia-heliotropism of, 640 drooping of, by frost, 400 response in ordinary, 61 Lepidium, heliotropic response of, 608 Light, action of, on autonomous torsion, 669 9 2? 32 29 9° on longitudinal growth, 572, 573, 574 on non-growing radial organ, 571 on pulsation of Desmodium, 690, 691 on pulvinated organ, 570 on subtonic tissue, 569 ues eee induced by, 84 directive versus non-directive action of, 634 diverse movements induced by, 565 effect on periodic reversals, 692 induction of torsion by, 659 multiple response induced by, 303, 584, 604, 634 orientating action of, 587, 697 supposed specific differences in the action of, 567 ;, . transmitted action of, 606 Light and gravitation, incomplete analogy between the actions of, 597 See Heliotropic response Longitudinal response in india-rubber, 12 3 in muscle, 12 es in plants, 13, 76-80 Lysimachia Nummularia, heliotropic response of, 622, 624 Mangifera ndica, response of, 653 Marchantia hallus, action of light on, 622 Mechanical response, by curling, 91 ” 99 by uncurling, 89 effect of chemical agents on, 131-137 », temperature on, 142-146 favourable conditions for exhibition of, 48 of Allium peduncle, 87 of Artocarpus, 55 of Biophytum, see Biophytum. of Mimosa, see Mimosa. of ordinary leaves, 61, 653, 654 similarity of, in pulvinus, pulvinoid and growing organ, 534 to artificial turgidity-variation, 45 to suctional activity, 399 two types of, 46 Michelia Champaca, chemotropic response of, 548 = INDEX Vil Michelia Champaca, galvanotropic response of, 559 Microscope Recorder, 599 Mimosa, death-point of, 153 duplication of rigor-point in, 179 effect of anzesthetics on excitability of, 135 > a” 99 39 9 be) 99 ” ‘3 3:9 darkness on the sensibility of, 22, 50 electrotonus on excitability of, 235 ether on conductivity of, 229 excessive turgor on the sensibility of, 49 fatigue on response of, 109 erectile response of, to suction, 426 excitatory versus plasmolytic reaction, 551, 552 fatigue-reversal in, 109, 113, 114 magnification of response of, 51 mechanical response of, 21, 22, 268 nyctitropic movement of, 681 polar effects on, 194, 197, 204, 208, 210 response of, to artificial variation of turgor, 46 99 ” to direct stimulation, 531 to electric radiation, 619 to indirect stimulation, 532 to light, 631, 63 to salt solution, 551, 552 thermo-mechanical record of, 172 torsional response of, 658, 659 Mohl on heliotropic insensitiveness of certain tendrils, 611 Molecular model, 217 Mono-polar method, 193 Morograph, 167 Motile areas in Artocarpus, 59 excitability, earlier abolition than conductivity, 112 ” 3 be) effect of anzesthetics on, 229 versus conductivity, 227, 228, 229 », receptivity, 226 Multiple response to constant stimulus: in inorganic substance, 39, 575 29 59 »5 in plant, 300, 301, 302, 303, 574, 604 5 a in retina, 301 to strong stimulus: electrical, 279 a5 - electrotactile, 280 3 $5 mechanical, 283 . A visual, 286 comparison of various types of, 432 continuity of, with autonomous, 291 cyclic variation of, in Bzophytum, 285 oO < in frog’s heart, 351 33 », in growth, 417, 424 $5 in visual impulse, 288 rpdtetan of, by light, 303, 584, 604, 634 merging of, into continuous, 586 776 PLANT RESPONSE Musa paradisiaca, response of, 68, 616 Muscarin, effect of, on cardiac pulsation, 4 NECTAR, excretion of, 398 Neméc on statolithic theory, 494 Noll on anomalous plasmolytic movement, 550 Nyctitropic movement, of Azophytum petiole, 680 As aS of AZimosa petiole, 681 oe 3 supposed distinction between heliotropic and, 677 OLD tissues, excitatory reaction in, 57 Oltmanns on heliotropic response of Lepzdium, 608 Optic Lever Recorder, 5 Oryza sativa, direct and indirect effects of stimulus on, 463 BS ;» diurnal variation of growth of, 473 55 5, effect of electrification of soil on growth of, 560 Osmotic versus excitatory action: on ascent of sap, 383 ” ” ” ” on growth, 485 a3 A s, on mechanical response, 549, 550, 551, 552 Grate diurnal sleep of, 633 PARA-HELIOTROPISM, 621 Paramecia, chemotaxis of, 699 oP galvanotaxis of, 698 = thermotaxis of, 698 Particles, response to unilateral pressure of, 496 Passiflora, death-discoloration of petals of, 183 aS effect of age on response, 79 e ;, season on response, 80 oe response of filament, 78 . “ tendril, 89, 611 os writhing movement of tendril of, 92 Perceptive region, of Artocarpus leaf, 60 e 3, of Desmodium leaflet, 588, 650 ee 5 Of leaflet of Erythrina indica, 651 Jp 3, true; 544 versus responding organ, 542, 588 eriadic after-effect, 683, 685 33 Hf factors determining, 468 » fatigue, 108 5» groupings: of growth-pulsation, 417, 425 a 3 of mechanical response, 286 5, reversals in Desmodium under light, 692 », Variation of Desmodium pulsation, 341, 470 se re of growth-rate, 470, 473, 474 By Rs. of transpiration, 472 Pertz, D., on influence of alternating stimulation, 684 Pfeffer on action of light, 566 5, on chemo-taxis, 599 INDEX 797 Pfeffer on effect of light on autonomous response, 690 »> on excitatory contraction in Cyzeree, 69 »» on expulsion of water from excited pulvinus of AZzmosa, 254 y» on geotropic sensitiveness of root-tip, 542 »» on radial pressure theory of geotropism, 494 Pfliiger on law of polar excitation in animal tissue, 200 Philanthus urinaria, response of, 44, 271 Photonasty, 621 Phototaxis, 696 Physical phenomena, merging of, into chemical, 122 Plagiotropic stem, daily periodic movement of, 627 “ », response of, to light, 624, 625, 626 oF 55 a to thermal shocks, 85 Plant-chamber, 16 Plasmolytic action, anomalous, 549 Poison, abolition of inorganic response by, 40 », effect of, on autonomous pulsation, 326 “7 », on growth, 486 x », on mechanical response, 132 - », on suctional response, 386 »» modifying action of constitution on the effect of, 322, 487 »> Opposite effects of dose on action of, 488 Polar action of currents: effect of fatigue on, 208 ” ” ” ” high Mer ys 207 >» » ” 3 low E.M.F., 195 ” ” ” ” moderate E.M.F., 197 » » 9 indirect effect of, 558 ” ots) 3 laws of, in plant, 199, 207, 558 ” 9 5 localised effect of, on pulvinus, 554 », effects of currents: localised action on pulvinus, 554 99 2 oo on death-response, 186, 207 ” » » on glow-response, 211 ” ” »» on growth, 557, 558, 559, 560 ” ” x on mechanical response, 193-206 Porana paniculata, autonomous torsional response of, 670 Pressure, internal hydrostatic : irregular variation of, 395 “4 aa x positive and negative, 393 Protozoa, polar effects in, 205 Pseudo-conduction, 251 Pulsation, discrete nature of, in Desmodium, 340, 341, 431 oe intermittent human, 288 es: » In Biophytum, 289 Pulse, healthy adult and senile, 3 », human, nitrite of amyl on, 4 Pulvinus and pulvinoid, 54 RADIATION, discrimination from temperature effect, 615 a effect of electric, on AZiizosa, 619 3 <3 thermal, 615-617 778 PLANT RESPONSE Receptivity, variation of, 461 ss ¥. by anesthetics, 224 _ 55 versus conductivity, 226 Recorder, Crescographic, 414, 416 5 Demonstration, 6 55 Electrical, 33 5 Magnetically controlled, 568-588 5 Microscopic, 519 a Optic Lever, 6, 16 ss Spiral-Spring, 26 eS Suctional, 340 Recti-petality, theory of, 593 Refractory period, 246, 273 Regional death, thermographs of, 183-186 Relaxation, anomalous meaning of, 339 Response, different types of, 123 i identity of, in mature and growing organ, 435 = relation of, to stimulus, 95, 96, 97 a similarities of, between motile and growing organs, 437 See Chemotactic-, Chemotropic-, Death-, Differential-, Galvanotactic-, Gal- vanotropic-, Geotropic-, Growth-, Heliotropic-, Inorganic-, Mechanical-, Phototropic-, and Suctional-. Responsiveness in matter, universality of, 40 Retina, multiple excitation of, 286 Rhythmic tissue, similarity of response in plant and animal, 345, 346, 348, 353 Rhythmicity, cause of, 309 Rigor, duplication of, 178 Rigor mortis, 153 Robinia, diurnal sleep movement of, 628 Root and shoot, no polar difference between, 512 », difference of effect between excitation at tip or growing region, 513 55 electric response, on direct stimulation of growing region, 518 = f 53 on stimulation of tip, 517 RKoot-tip, effect of amputation of, on excitability, 540 3 localisation of geotropic sensitiveness at, 539, 542 SACHs on action of light on J/archantia thallus, 622 », on characteristic effects of light, 567 », on heliotropic response of Zvopcolum majus, 623 Seasonal variation of conductivity and excitability, 474 r, », of contractility, 79 ”» », of growth, 474 id », Of heliotropic response, 623 Selenium, response of, 39 Semi-automatism, 289 Sensitiveness, abolition by over-turgidity, 58 “r modification in darkness, 22, 50 9 wide range in plants, 43 Sesbanta coccineum, death-discoloration of, 183 INDEX 779 Shoshungraph, 364-371 Ao record by Hydraulic Balance Method, 368 ne 5 Hydrostatic Balance Method, 368, 375 Simultaneous mechanical and electrical records, 34 Sinapis nigra, effect of diffuse light on growth of, 572 of », negative heliotropic response of root of, 601 os »» positive, dia-, and negative heliotropic response of, 609 ‘5 »» positive heliotropic response of seedling of, 592 Sodium carbonate, effect of solution of, on growth, 481 », chloride, effect of solution of, on growth, 484 ” ” ” » on mechanical response, 551 > ”» >» » on suction, 384 Sphygmograph, 3 Spirogyra, thermo-mechanical curve of, 170 Stahl on phototaxis, 697 ‘ Staircase ’ response in galena, 121 D Bs in muscle, 122 o #5 in plant, 104, 121, 122 Stimulation, direct effect of, on growth, 434, 435, 436, 438 9 35 “5 on mechanical response, 47, 420 An effect of unilateral direct, on growth-curvature, 527, 529 be a os indirect, on growth-curvature, 525, 528, 529 a indirect effect of, on growth, 438 3 ms rp on mechanical response, 47, 420 be minimally effective, 26 oe re of variation of, at different temperatures, 27 Stimulus, additive effect of, 94 AF and response, relation between, 95, 96, 97 55 a .e modified by tonic condition, 465 fe bifurcation of effect of, 127 a detection of latent, 459 ts different modes of transformation of, 123-126 ap supposed explosive effect of, 454 Strasburger on action of light on autonomous movements, 690 a0 », Of poison on ascent of sap, 351 #5 oF on phototaxis, 697 Suction, automatic record of variation in the rate of, 371 » internal pressure due to, 393, 394, 395 », mechanical response to, 399 Suctional response, effect of cold water on, 373, 376, 379 ” ” », KNO, solution on, 383 » 99 », NaCl solution on, 384 ” ie 3» poisonous solution on, 386 » ” »> Warm water on, 375, 378, 380 » » excitatory nature of, 382 » » 3 versus plasmolytic action on, 383 x 7. persistence of, when root killed, 381 Sugar, chemotactic effect of, 699 », chemotropic effect of, 549 780 PLANT RESPONSE Sugar, effect of solution on growth, 482 ; en Sunflower, impressed periodic movement in, 685 Tamarindus indica, diurnal variation in the rate of growth of, 474 initiation of growth in, by stimulus, 425 - », response to thermal radiation, 617 Temperature and growth, relation between: determination by Continuous Method, 446 Fs = NaHS - determination by Discon- tinuous Method, 442 determination by Method of Balance, 450 : 3 ie i determination by Method of Excitatory Response, 452 and rhythmic frequency, relation between, 349 effect of maximum, on arrest of Desmodium pulsation, 432 29 be) 99 bed 99 3 =? es 5 on growth-arrest, 431 3 »» on cardiac pulsation, 333, 334 33 ; on Desmodium pulsation, 332-338 53 ;» on intensity of minimally effective stimulus, 27 - 55 on latent period, 141, 268 3 ie on response, 140-146 re optimum, of growth, 449, 451 FA variation, effect on growth, 615 Tesla current, effect of, on growth, 619 Tetanus in muscle, 98 - in plant, 99 oa incapability of, in rhythmic tissue, 340 Thermal radiation, discrimination between effect of, and temperature, 615 = nn growth-curvature induced by, 616, 617 Thermo-crescent curve, 447 Thermographs, development of, 184 is of regional death, 183-186 Thermo-mechanical curve, 160 Thermo-taxis, 698 Thermo-tonic minimum in Azophylum, 305 ss ne in Desmodium, 305 Tip of root or shoot, electric response on stimulation of, 517, 518 a 9 mechanical response on stimulation of, 525, 526, 601, 606 a effect on excitability on amputation of, 541 Tonic condition, modifying effect of, on relation between stimulus and response, 465 3 », true meaning of, 307, 313 Torsion, autonomous, 666 effect of electric current on, 670 gravity on, 671 7" a 5 light on, 669 mS rp re temperature on, 667 Torsional response, laws of, 662 fr a of compound strip, 660 99 > 39 39 99 INDEX 781 Torsional response, of Desmodium, 657 5 “5 of Erythrina indica, 661 & 30 of AZimosa, 658, 659 Transmission of heliotropic excitation, 604 Transpiration, automatic record of variation of, 371, 472 SP effect of dissolved salt on, 383 Transverse conduction of stimulation, 519, 528, 529, 532 Tropeolum majus, heliotropic response in : seasonal variation of, 623 a - > 5 variation of, with intensity of stimulus, 623, 626 Turgidity, positive and negative variation: electrical indication of, 37, 41 ey ee Az “e induced changes of rate of growth by, 426, 435 3 = F 53 mechanical indication of, 37, 41 $5 response by artificial, 45 Ulothrix, phototaxis of, 697 Uniform response, 104 Uriclis Lily, apogeotropic response of scape of, 500, 505 “é periodic fatigue in the response of style of, 108 VARIATION, induction of, by external forces, 750 Verworn on polar excitation of protozoa, 200 Vines on autonomous epi- and hypo-nasty, 643 »» On protoplasmic irritability, 53 Vintschgau on velocity of nervous impulse, 246 Visual impulse, multiple, 286, 300 Vitalism, 740 Vitis, heliotropic response of tendril of, 611 Vochting on.theory of recti-petality, 594 WATER, excretion of, 396 PRINTED BY SPOTTISWOODE AND CO, LTD., NEW-STREET SQUARE LONDON LIFB IN. PLANTS. Plant Response as a Means of Physiologteal | Investigation. By Jagadis Chunder Bose, M.A.; D.Sc. Professor, Presidency College, Caleutta. New York: Longmans, Green & Co. $7. Except to those who are acquainted with the previous work of this author, the pres- ent treatise may come as a surprise. a substantial octavo volume of more than 500 pages, devoted to the elucidation and illustration of a single thesis. Although thi thesis is here given in many forms and stated in connection with numerous associ- ated topics, it is essentially simple in its ' Outline. It is this: the plant is a machine; its movements in response to external stim- uli, though apparently various, are ulti- mately reducible to a fundamental unity of reaction. But the movements here referred to are not alone the obvious movements which the eye readily sees, as in the case of the sensitive plant and in that of the twiners. Everybody knows to-day that all young parts of plants possess a limited power of movement in response to certain external stimuli, but to this’ interesting chapter in the life-history of plants Pro- fessor Bose adds others which are of éyen greater interest. In general, it may be said that, by means of ingenious deli- cate instruments which exaggerate the slightest motions at any spot, he has been able to demonstrate that even the oldest tissues of a plant, so long as they are liv- ing, are capable of responding in a marked degree to certain external stimuli. By the employment of a beam of light reflected from a mirror attached to a multiplying lever, and falling on a recording drum cov- ered with photographic paper or film, he records the amplitude of the very minutest movements of these responses. Now it so happens that many of these responses are exceedingly complex and exhibit confused | effects, due to internal energy and external stimulus respectively. Professor Bose be- lieves that he has been able to disentangle these complex phenomena of combined ac- tion. After this disentangling and exact diserimination have been effected, he ar- rives at the conclusion that “‘there is no physiological response given by the most highly organized animal tissue that is not also to be met with in the plant.” A special feature distinguishing this treatise from many of its class is the pre- sentation, at the end of every chapter, of @ summary which gi'ves in a few short sen- Bae Posh UY. Suk, 6 ONAN It is, “Here are studied such matters as fatigue : in’plants, the effect of anzstheties, and the , aie tences the ‘gubstauce of the synopses are often found treatises and are most acceptable, not to the student, but to the casual r The treatise considers, first, simple | sponse, and deals with the universal sensitiveness in plants, This is ni ‘followed by a consideration of the di fic jtion of response under various condi death-spasm in plants. Next comes a 4 devoted to excitability and conducti ity, to} reversal of normal polar effects in ivi tissues, to electrotonus, and the latent “fefractory” period. Under multiple autonomous response the author g together the results of his studies on perature and rhythmic responses in plants, introductory to the larger subject ey. different tropisms. But, before taking ‘up, he intercalates two chapters of found interest, namely, (1) the Ascent Sap, and (2) Growth. His discussion of former topic is not wholly satisfactory, t it is extremely suggestive. After the isms are examined in most of their 1 tions, he ventures on a general surve the whole matter, in the course of W he has something to say about variat induced by external forces that th light on his views of Darwinism. From the foregoing, it will be seen no summary of the work can be given wt does not take up all of the author’s ow summaries and his concluding part (Ix as well. The whole work must be stu as a unit, and with strict reference to | thesis on which it is based. Nevertk there are many incidental results Wi possess interest for all readers, such, instance, as the effect of alcohol plants in causing temporary exaltatio response, followed by depression and p tracted period of recovery. The style Datura Alba was used inthese experime: If the alcohol vapor was blown away fresh air substituted, the tissues slowly covered their normal excitability, - But instead of alcohol vapor, dilute ashe lution was applied, the depressing © was immediate and very great. There ig nothing in the treatise to. gest that the work is not the ou a physiological laboratory in Europe and from the hands of a physiologist in & many or England, But the author is an Indian, much of whose experimental has been carried on under tropical | tions. The greater part of. his hated has been found in India, where, as in tropical countries, vegetation does not the long periods of hibernation characte tic of the temperate zones. There have been at his command a ‘ nf 3 ti poate rely, will Bus- has ile Tes lating wad) is oversy. It has 1b 1 gists, | but a - > a1