RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN LOCUS OF CONTROL AND COPING STRATEGIES IN COMMUNITY MENTAL HEALTH OUTPATIENTS BY MARGIE K. STEVENSON A DISSERTATION PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE CGUNE Cr THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHv UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA ] 982 1 This dissertation is dedicated to my father, Leon Mayatte, who taught me to value education and achievement and to my mother, Lucille Mayatte, who taught me to value myself. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I wish to express my appreciation to my doctoral committee chairman, Dr. Ted Landsman, for his patient guidance and for setting an example of the highest professional standards. Appreciation is also extended to the members of my committee. Dr. David Suchman deserves special thanks for his help in generating a research topic and for his awareness of and responsiveness to my need for emotional support throughout this work. Dr. Ruth Alexander has provided an excellent role model of a professional woman who has accomplished much in her field without sacrificing her femininity. I am also indebted to Dr. James Algina, a very competent statis- tician, for his technical assistance and for his efforts to commu- nicate his expertise in a manner that was meaningful to me. My deepest love and appreciation are extended to my wonderful husband for his many personal sacrifices and his unfailing encouragement. Without his understanding and support this dissertation would not have been possible. m TABLE OF CONTENTS Page ACKNOWLEDGMENTS in LIST OF TABLES vii ABSTRACT vii i CHAPTER I: INTRODUCTION 1 Rationale for the Study 5 Research Hypotheses 6 Definitions of Terms 6 CHAPTER II: REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE 10 Field Studies of Denial -Sensi tization in Surgical Patients 12 Experimental Studies of Denial -Sensi ti zation in Surgical Patients 13 Laboratory Studies Conceptualizing Coping Along Denial-Sensitization Dimension 14 Summary of Research Conceptualizing Coping as Denial-Sensitization 17 Atheoretical Studies of Coping with Naturally Occurring Stressors 19 Evidence for the Adaptive Value of Strategies Which Incorporate Denial 22 Strategies Which Combine Denial and Action- Oriented Behaviors ■- '■ Studies Which Emphasize the Effectiveness of Action-Oriented Strategies Summary of Studies of Coping with Naturally Occurring Events 22. ... 28 Determinants of Coping Behavior 29 iv Sex Differences in Coping Strategy 31 Summary of Male-Female Differences in Coping 34 SES Differences in Coping Style 35 Locus of Control as a Mediator of Responses to Stressful Events 37 Field Studies Relating Locus of Control to Coping Strategies 38 Experimental Studies Relating Response to Aversive Events to Locus of Control 43 Summary of Research Relating Locus of Control to Coping Strategy 45 Research on Coping in Mental Health Clients 45 Null Hypotheses 50 CHAPTER III: METHOD 52 Subjects 52 Instruments 54 Procedure 64 Data Analysis 66 Statistical Analysis 74 CHAPTER IV: RESULTS 76 Direct Action Responses 78 Defensive Responses 80 Functional Somatic-Oriented Responses 80 Potentially Dysfunctional Somatic-Oriented Responses --Type A 80 Potentially Dysfunctional Somatic-Oriented Responses --Tyoe B °' CHAPTER V: DISCUSSION 82 Exploratory Data 88 General izabil ity of Findings 92 Implications for Counseling 92 Limitations of the Study and Suggestions for Future Research 94 Summary of Study 95 APPENDIX A: QUESTIONNAIRES 99 APPENDIX B: DEMOGRAPHIC DATA FORM 109 APPENDIX C: INFORMED CONSENT FORM I'll REFERENCES 112 BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH 119 VI LIST OF TABLES Table Page 1. Demographic Characteristics of Research Subjects 53 2. Pilot Study Varimax Rotated Factor Matrix 62 3. Varimax Rotated Factor Matrix 67 4. Subscales Used in Hypothesis Testing 70 5. Correlations Between Locus of Control and Coping Responses 77 6. Relationship Between Control Variables and Correlational Variables 79 7. Mean Scores on Locus of Control and Coping Response Scales 85 VI i Abstract of Dissertation Presented to the Graduate Council of the University of Florida in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN LOCUS OF CONTROL AND COPING STRATEGIES IN COMMUNITY MENTAL HEALTH OUTPATIENTS By Margie K. Stevenson December, 1982 Chairman: Theodore Landsman Major Department: Counselor Education A study of the relationship between locus of control and mecha- nisms for coping with stressful events was conducted with 40 male and 60 female community mental health outpatient subjects. Responses to stressful situations were measured by the Coping Response Inventory, a 24-item rating scale developed by the researcher, based on coping response categories designated by Richard Lazarus as direct action, defensive and somatic-oriented. Locus of control was assessed by the Adult Nowicki-Strickland Internal -External Control Scale. A partial correlation analysis was conducted to determine the linear relation- ship between locus of control and each of five coping response cate- gories, while controlling for sex and socioeconomic status (SES). A statistically significant relationship was found between locus of control and choice of coping responses. As internal i ty increased, choice of direct action and functional somatic-oriented responses viii increased and choice of one of two types of potentially dysfunctional somatic-oriented responses decreased. Externality was found to relate positively to choice of defensive and one of two types of potentially dysfunctional somatic-oriented responses and negatively to choice of direct action and functional somatic-oriented responses. The control variables, sex and SES, contributed little to the relationship between locus of control and coping strategies, although there was a tendency for persons of higher SES to use more direct action responses and for males to have higher SES than females. The rather low correlation co- efficients, which ranged between .20 and .40, indicated that although a person's belief concerning ability to influence outcomes is a factor related to choice of responses to stressful events, it is not a major factor. Other variables not included in the hypotheses of the study which appeared to influence coping style were marital status, age and the type of problem with which a person was coping. The results of this study support Lazarus' theory that coping is a complex process which includes some combination of responses which act directly upon the stressor, responses which function to distort awareness of the stressor and reduce threat and responses which alleviate the dysphoric somatic accompaniments to coping with stressful events. IX CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION Since the definition of stress as a biological concept by Selye (1956), social scientists from the disciplines of anthropology, psy- chology, sociology and medicine have amassed an impressive body of research data which established a relationship between adversive life experiences and breakdown of physical and psychological functioning. In addition to the part that they play in psychosomatic diseases, stressful life events have been shown to be related to many somatic disorders including heart disease, childhood leukemia and fractures; as well as to the psychological disorders of acute schizophrenia, neurosis, depression and suicide attempts (Andrews, Tennant, Hewson & Vaillant, 1978; Dohrenwend & Dohrenwend , 1978). Yet, despite exposure of all people to the stresses of living, which might include loss of a loved one, changes in living or work situation, financial reversals, injury, frustration or failure, only a relatively small percentage of people succumb to physical or psychological impairment. It was this knowledge that exposure to stressors alone was almost never sufficient to explain onset of illness which has led to interest in identifying those factors which mediate the impact of adversive experiences and allow the individual to adapt to or cope with demanding circumstances (Rabkin & Struening, 1976). For the purposes of this paper Selye's definition of stress as "the state manifested by a specific syndrome which consists of all the nonspecifically-inauced changes within a biologic system" (1956, p. 54) 1 2 will be adopted. This definition views stress as a physiological re- sponse of the system which is separate from those events which cause it and is of interest here primarily as a motivator to relieve the accompanying discomfort and reestablish the body's homeostasis. The causal events or stimuli to which stress is a response are re- ferred to as stressors or stressful situations and include any number of nonspecific conditions which require change or adaptation. Al- though stressors may be purely physical demands upon the body, psycho- logical stressors are of special importance because of the frequency with which individuals are faced with challenging emotional stimuli in the course of their daily lives (Selye, 1976). While certain events such as the death of a loved one or loss of one's job are typically considered stressful by most people, there is great diversity in the degree to which the same event is considered stressful and in responses to such events. Individuals may respond quite differently to the same event depending on whether it is per- ceived as threatening, challenging, or of little significance to them. Threat, therefore, is not an inherent property of a stimulus but depends rather on the appraisal of its personal significance and of one's potential for mastery (Lazarus, 1964). This appraisal, which is influenced by many factors including characteristics of the stimulus, the environment and the person, determines not only the manner in which the event will be interpreted, but to a large degree, the choice of re- sponse. If an event is perceived as a serious threat to self and one's ability to reverse it is judged to be inadequate, the individual is likely to respond quite differently from one who sees the event as a challenge and feels that the capacity for mastery is high (Mechanic, 3 1962). This process of assessing the threat value of an event and formulating a response to it is the essence of coping. Knowledge of this process and its determinants is integral to the understanding of behavior in the face of stressful circumstances. Coping has been variously defined as "things that people do to avoid being harmed by life strains" (Pearlin & Schooler, 1978, p. 2); "efforts to deal with some new, and often problematic, situation or encounter or to deal in some new way with an old problem" (Perlman, 1975, p. 213) and "behavior which has consequences relevant to the situational demands" (Mechanic, 1962, p. 52). Lazarus and Launier (1978) defined coping as "efforts ... to manage (i.e., master, tolerate, reduce, minimize) environmental and internal demands, and conflicts among them, which tax or exceed a person's resources" (p. 311). The Lazarus and Launier conceptualization, which has been adopted for this study, regards coping as the product of a cognitive appraisal process whereby both the properties of the stimulus and the individual's assessment of his/her potential for mastery enter into defining a situation as stressful and formulating a response to it. Lazarus (1977) defined three types of responses to situations ap- praised as stressful. Direct action or instrumental responses are attempts to influence or change the harmful nature of the stimulus, defensive responses are attempts to deny or ameliorate the individual's perception of the event and somatic-oriented responses are efforts aimed at reducing the dysphoric somatic conditions which accompany the body's physiological stress response. For the purposes of this study somatic-oriented responses have been divided into those which are functional and those which are potentially dysfunctional. 4 Data from field studies of naturally occurring stressors and from experimental manipulations in laboratory settings have contributed greatly to the understanding of the coping process and the many vari- ables related to it. In recent years focus has shifted to assessment and analysis of the mechanisms or dispositions which underlie coping, especially those which determine the cognitive appraisal of threat and the choice of coping strategy (Lazarus, 1974). One variable which may be viewed as an organizing or guiding principle in this regard is the individual's belief concerning the ability to control the course of life and to intervene effectively in environmental events. This belief or expectancy regarding the nature of the causal relationship between one's own behavior and its consequences has been called locus of control (Rotter, 1966). Individuals who believe that outcomes are contingent upon their own actions are labelled internals and persons who attribute what happens to them to luck, fate or powerful others are referred to as externals. Internally controlled individuals, who have a strong sense of personal efficacy, are more likely to initiate attempts to alter negative circumstances than externally controlled persons who are likely to respond in a manner consistent with their belief that very little which they do directly results in an effect (Wolk and Kurtz, 1975). That locus of control can function as a moderator of the aver- siveness of events has been shown in experimental studies with humans as well as animals (Lefcourt, 1973). These studies provide evidence that the illusion of control, even if not exercised, results in less physiological arousal as a result of exposure to aversive stimuli than when such control is lacking. Rationale for the Study The purpose of the present study is to identify coping strategies being used by clients seeking counseling in a community mental health facility and to relate those strategies to locus of control orien- tation. It is hypothesized that effective coping strategies, greater use of direct action and functional somatic-oriented responses, are related to perception of life events as internally controlled and that ineffective coping strategies, greater reliance on defensive and poten- tially dysfunctional somatic-oriented responses, are related to per- ception of life events as externally controlled. Confirmation of such a relationship, although it would not establish causation, would provide the clinician with information of value in developing therapeutic interventions for persons who are having difficulty coping with current circumstances. The study of the coping process and its determinants is of par- ticular relevance to the mental health clinician who provides services to persons who often enter the mental health delivery system over- whelmed by an event or series of events with which their usual coping strategies are inadequate to deal (Perlman, 1975). The client's very presence in the initial interview is evidence of a search for a new approach to handling present circumstances. Since the seeking of pro- fessional services is usually a rather late step in the process of problem resolution after other attempts have failed (Overbeck, 1977), the counseling interview offers a unique opportunity to study the attempts which clients have made and continue to make in negotiating the stressful situations with which they are actively engaged. In addition to the knowledge to be gained from such an assessment in 6 furthering the understanding of the coping process, such an inquiry could be very helpful to the clinician in planning effective treatment. One of the major purposes of therapy is to help clients learn to recognize and understand the relationship between their behavior and their difficulties and to accept responsibility for initiating changes which will improve their condition (Baker, 1979). Therapeutic inter- ventions for increasing awareness of personal responsibility and potential for having an effect on one's life events are outlined by Connolly (1980) and Stensrud and Stensrud (1981). If locus of control orientation is related to choice of coping strategies the most ef- fective intervention might be one which combines techniques for chang- ing perception of control with exploration and implementation of more effective coping responses. Research Hypotheses HA,A As internality increases choice of direct action and functional somatic-oriented responses increases. HA,D As internality increases choice of defensive and potentially 1 B dysfunctional somatic-oriented responses decreases. HA9fl As externality increases choice of direct action and functional somatic-oriented responses decreases. HA As externality increases choice of defensive and potentially 2B dysfunctional somatic-oriented responses increases. Definitions of Terms Stress Stress is "the state manifested by a specific syndrome which consists of all the nonspecif ical ly-induced changes within a biologic system" (Selye, 1956, p. 54). Stressor A stressor is a causal event or stimulus to which stress is a response. Stressors may include any number of nonspecific conditions which require change or adaptation and may be either physical or psychological in nature. Physical stressors may include physical in- jury, disease or exposure to noxious chemicals, extreme temperature or aversive noise levels. Examples of psychological stressors include loss of a loved one, changes in one's job or living situation, finan- cial reversals, frustration or failure. Coping Coping is defined as ". . . efforts ... to manage (i.e., master, tolerate, reduce, minimize) environmental and internal demands, and conflicts among them, which tax or exceed a person's resources" (Lazarus & Launier, 1978, p. 311). Coping efforts include direct action responses, defensive responses, functional somatic-oriented responses and potentially dysfunctional somatic-oriented responses. Direct Action or Instrumental Responses Direct action or instrumental responses are coping efforts which are aimed directly at influencing or eliminating the harmful nature of the stimulus. Direct action responses include preparation to meet danger, attack against the harmful agent or evasion to avoid contact when threatened by an agent which is overwhelmingly powerful or dangerous. Direct action responses are most often related to positive outcomes in managing stressful situations. Examples include seeking information, analyzing the situation, exploring alternatives, con- sulting with others and developing a plan of action. The terms direct action and instrumental responses are used interchangeably. Defensive Responses Defensive responses are attempts to deny or ameliorate one's per- ception of an event so that threat is reduced. Threat, however, is reduced only in the mind of the individual as no action has been taken to influence the stressor. Defensive strategies are considered costly in that they distort reality and interfere with seeking adaptive alter- natives. If evidence of the stressor increases, the defensive strat- egy may become ineffective and the threat of harm may be reinstated. Defensive responses include use of denial, projection, intellectu- al ization or rationalization and may occur at conscious and unconscious levels. Examples include ignoring the problem, deciding that it does not really have an important effect, focusing on the concerns of others and believing that things will take care of themselves. Somatic-oriented Responses Somatic-oriented responses are efforts directed toward reducing the dysphoric somatic conditions which accompany the process of antici- pating and responding to stressful events. Somatic accompaniments may be experienced as increased anxiety, loss of appetite, sleeplessness, impaired concentration or depressed mood state. Somatic-oriented responses help to control those discomforts which interfere with adap- tive responding without distorting or denying the significance of the stressor. For the purposes of this study somatic-oriented responses have been divided into functional and potentially dysfunctional re- sponses. Functional somatic-oriented responses. Functional somatic- oriented responses are those which relieve somatic discomfort and which have the potential for increasing present and future coping 9 capacity. Examples include engaging in physical exercise, gardening, doing handwork or carpentry, reading or listening to music. Potentially dysfunctional somatic-oriented responses. Potential- ly dysfunctional somatic-oriented responses are those responses which temporarily relieve somatic discomfort but which have the potential for becoming additional sources of stress themselves. Examples include drinking alcohol, smoking, taking tranquilizers or sleeping pills. Locus of Control Locus of control is a generalized expectancy which operates across a large number of situations regarding the causal relationship be- tween one's own behavior and its consequences. Internal locus of control is the belief that outcomes or reinforcements are contingent upon one's own actions, while external locus of control is the belief that reinforcements are attributable to luck, fate or powerful others. CHAPTER II REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE The study of the ways in which people cope with stressful life situations has been of interest to researchers in a variety of disci- plines for the past 25 to 30 years. Earliest investigations often took the form of atheoretical field studies which observed how indi- viduals coped with naturally occurring stressors such as being se- verely burned (Hamburg, Hamburg & deGoza, 1953), stricken with polio- melytis (Visotsky, Hamburg, Goss & Lebovits, 1961), undergoing major surgery (Abram, 1965; Janis, 1958), losing a child through leukemia (Chodoff, Friedman & Hamburg, 1964) or taking Ph.D. qualifying exami- nations (Mechanic, 1962). From these early observations in the field came the knowledge that the defense mechanisms played a major role in the way individuals coped with stressful events. The tendency to use denial and isolation of affect was found to be a typical component of adaptive behavior in a variety of situations which represented threat to life or to self-esteem. Janis (1958), in observing the coping be- havior of patients anticipating major surgery, found that many showed no apprehension prior to surgery but felt calm and invulnerable, appar- ently denying or minimizing the possible dangers and threatening impli- cations of their surgery. Along with those who typically employed de- nial, Janis identified two other groups — those who were extremely appre- hensive and who felt highly vulnerable to bodily damage and those who were moderately fearful prior to surgery. The tendency to be either 10 11 extremely sensitive to the threatening aspects of a situation or to deny them completely was found to relate to negative postsurgical outcomes (Janis, 1958). Both styles appeared to prevent the individual from developing strategies to cope with the surgery and the post- surgical period. The group which had a moderate level of arousal con- cerning surgery asked for and received realistic information about what was going to happen to them and their convalescence was less difficult. In addition, this group of individuals exhibited fewer negative psychological reactions such as anger or resentfulness against their caretakers. The identification of these three coping styles spurred interest in the study of the polarities of denial and sensi- tization. Two instruments were developed which measured the dimension of denial-sensitization. Byrne (1961) constructed the Repression- Sensitization (RS) Scale from Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI) items. The RS scale dichotomized individuals into those who responded to threatening situations by avoiding or denying the threat (repressors) and those who responded in similar circum- stances by becoming alert to and seeking information about the threat- ening nature of the situation (sensitizers). Goldstein (1959) devised a sentence completion test which divided subjects into three coping styles depending on the extent to which their responses reflected a move toward or away from potentially threatening emotions. Those with personalized, specific feeling-acknowledgments were defined as copers; those with stereotyped denial or distancing of feelings, as avoiders and those who emphasized neither style, as nonspecific defenders (Andrew, 1970). Copers were similar to Byrne's sensitizers in being 12 hyperalert to the threatening aspects of the environment, while avoiders were like repressors in denying those aspects (Lazarus, Averill S Opton, 1974). A number of studies using one or the other of these measures of denial-sensitization have appeared in the literature. Several of these studies were conducted with patients awaiting surgery and can be divided into two types — field studies which observed the relationship between coping strategy and postsurgical outcome and those which manip- ulated the situation experimentally in an attempt to influence post- surgical outcome. Field Studies of Denial-Sensitization in Surgical Patients Studies of this group correlated coping strategy with a recovery index which included number of days hospitalized postsurgical ly, number of patient-requested medications and number of minor compli- cations. The assumption was made that patients in each study were homogeneous with regard to seriousness of condition and surgical pro- cedure. In a study of male cancer patients considered by a physician to be similar in relation to medical condition, MacCornack (1979) found that copers did best on the recovery index, with nonspecific defenders next and avoiders having the poorest outcomes. In a second study of this type Cohen and Lazarus (1973) measured outcomes for 61 patients (22 male, 39 female) admitted for elective surgery. They found that copers showed slowest recovery, with more days hospitalized and greater frequency of minor complications, while nonspecific defenders and avoiders fared equally well. This study did not control for serious- ness of surgery, therefore, the results were confounded by this over- sight. A retrospective examination of the data showed that three of 13 the four most serious cases were in the coper group. This could account for their poorer recovery rates. Experimental Studies of Denial-Sensitization in Surgical Patients Three other studies of surgical patients which categorized sub- jects along the denial-sensitization coping dimension included an experimental manipulation. Andrew (1970) assessed outcomes for 40 males who had hernia surgery. Using Goldstein's test, patients were divided into copers, avoiders and nonspecific defenders and were pro- vided with audiotape information about the origin of hernia, the dangers of delaying surgery; the surgical procedure and its conse- quences. Of the three groups nonspecific defenders fared best, staying fewest days in the hospital and requesting less medication than either other group. There was no difference between copers and avoiders on days of hospitalization; however, avoiders required more medication. DeLong (1971) assessed the effect of providing audiotape information prior to surgery, which was either consistent or inconsistent with the patient's orientation toward threatening stimuli. For copers con- sistent information was considered to be specific information about the impending surgery and inconsistent information was general information about the hospital. The reverse was considered consistent/inconsistent for avoiders. As predicted, copers provided with specific informa- tion did best on the recovery index and on a subjective measure of recovery. Avoiders who had been given specific information made more postoperative complaints. Regardless of the type of information given, avoiders showed slower, more complicated recoveries. Ritter (1980) also found that providing information consistent with coping style 14 resulted in decreased presurgical anxiety for 50 (37 male, 13 female) patients scheduled for open-heart surgery, while no change in anxiety was noted for those given inconsistent information. Laboratory Studies Conceptualizing Coping Along Denial-Sensitization Dimension Laboratory studies have also used the tendency to approach or to avoid threatening stimuli as a dimension along which to study the effects of the experimental manipulation of stressors. Diskin, Gold- stein and Grencik (1977) divided 135 law enforcement officers into copers, avoiders and nonspecific defenders. After being further cate- gorized into high or low anxiety groups according to scores on the Trait-State Anxiety Scale, subjects were given one of three treatments prior to exposure to a stressful film. The film depicted workshop accidents in which workers were injured and which culminated in a scene where a man died after being impaled through the abdomen by a board. Subjects in two conditions were given specific information concerning the disturbing events in the film, after being designated to either relax alone for 10 minutes or to view a travelog about marine life. The third group was immediately exposed to the film without any informa- tion about its contents nor the opportunity to relax. Arousal (stress reaction), as measured by skin conductance and subjective self-report, was greatest in the condition where no information was provided prior to the film. Highest reactivity was observed in subjects who were categorized as high anxiety copers. These subjects were also rated lowest on job performance by their supervisors, while low anxiety avoiders and nonspecific defenders received the highest job ratings. In a design which assessed the relationship between coping style and exposure to failure, Carter (1974) divided 84 female 15 undergraduates into sensitizers, repressors and neutrals as measured by the Byrne RS scale. Half of each group had a successful experience with an anagram task and the other half experienced failure. Subjects filled out five rating scales designed to elicit their responses to the experience. Under conditions of failure, sensitizers reported more anxiety and greater dissatisfaction with their performance than either repressors or neutrals. A study by Houston and Hodges (1970) observed the effect of either threat of failure or electric shock on subjects categorized as deniers, neutrals or sensitizers (accentuators) . Rather than the paper and pencil instruments used by the investigators mentioned above, a be- havioral measure of coping tendency was developed based on discrepancy between subjective self-support and physiological measures of stress. One hundred and eight male introductory psychology students were given the digits backwards test from the Wechler Adult Intelligence Scale and then were assigned to one of three conditions. Subjects in one condition were told that they were doing well, subjects in a second condition were told that they were doing poorly and subjects in the third condition were told that they would be given an electric shock to study its effect on blood pressure. All subjects were then given the Affective Adjective Check List as a measure of subjective dis- comfort. Heart rate was recorded prior to and after the threat manip- ulation and used as an indicator of physiological arousal. Subjects who exhibited physiological arousal which was relatively greater than their reported subjective discomfort were designated deniers (tended to deny their arousal). Subjects who reported relatively greater subjective discomfort than they evidenced physiological arousal were 16 designated sensitizers or accentuators (accentuated their arousal). Those individuals who showed a minimal discrepancy between the two indicators were categorized as neutrals. Effectiveness of coping style was measured by monitoring performance on a second adminis- tration of the digits backwards test. The denial group performed best under both the threat of shock condition and the failure condition, with neutrals next and sensitizers doing worst in both of the condi- tions. These researchers concluded that denial was an effective strat- egy for these subjects when confronted with threat to ego (failure) or threat of pain. Averill and Rosenn (1972) devised a different type of operational definition of denial -sensitization for a study conducted with 80 male undergraduates anticipating and receiving electric shock. Sensiti- zation or vigilance was defined as choosing to listen for a warning signal during anticipation of shock, while denial was defined as choosing instead to listen to music. A second measure of the tendency to avoid or approach threatening stimuli was the subject's response to three avoidant and three vigilant types of statements indicating what he was thinking about during each trial. Each subject was exposed to three different levels of shock intensity. An avoidance response was available in one condition if subjects listened for the warning signal, while in another condition shock was delivered regardless of the coping style the subject adopted. Although vigilance increased as a function of shock intensity, especially among the avoidance group, a sizable portion (30% in the avoidance condition and 50% in the nonavoidance condition) chose a denial type of response even at the highest shock intensity. These deniers who chose to listen to music were more 17 physiologically aroused based on heart rate and galvanic skin re- sistance (GSR) than those who chose the vigilant strategy of listening for the preshock signal. Subjects who listened for the tone (sensi- tizers) showed least arousal in the condition where they could make a response to avoid the shock (avoidance condition). Another study which points to the tendency to adopt and per- sistently maintain a denial coping style in dealing with threat was reported by Niemela (1974). The GSR patterns of 15 male and 15 female subjects anticipating and receiving shock were monitored. The majority of the subjects showed anticipatory arousal prior to delivery of the shock, evidence that they were sensitized to the impending aversive event. However, six subjects showed no anticipation but exhibited a very high arousal response to the delivery of the shock. When inter- viewed later these subjects reported that they had denied the possi- bility of receiving the shock during the waiting period. A question- naire was also administered to assess everyday stress behavior. For those who tended to use denial in the experimental situation, a pat- tern emerged which was characterized by avoidance of disagreeable events and failure to undertake preparation. This pattern was in con- trast to that of subjects who were attuned to warnings and prepared themselves prior to the occurrence of a stressful event. Summary of Research Conceptualizing Coping as Denial-Sensitization The findings derived from these studies which conceptualized coping with stressful situations along an approach-avoidance dimension are admittedly inconsistent. These discrepancies may be due in some cases to failure to control relevant variables, as in the Cohen and 18 Lazarus (1973) study of surgical patients where the groups were not homogeneous with regard to seriousness of condition. However, despite the lack of agreement on some points, some consistent information can be extracted. These studies seem to confirm that there is a tendency for individuals to cope with a stressful situation by either avoiding or by being sensitized to and/or approaching its threatening aspects. These two styles are apparent whether coping is assessed by question- naire or through behavioral measures. Preferences for one style rather than another within a given situation appear to be persistent regardless of the consequences. Denial seems to be a viable mechanism when threat is not extremely high, such as in performing a task in the laboratory after being told that you are doing poorly, or awaiting shock which is never delivered. Its effectiveness in controlling arousal may not hold up when the threat actually materializes, as when the shock is delivered, and may result in a more severe reaction at that point than if a more vigilant strategy had been utilized (Niemela, 1974). The effectiveness with which police officers (Diskin et al . , 1977) utilized denial both in dealing with laboratory conditions of threat and in carrying out their duties, as evidenced by the high job performance ratings given avoiders by their supervisors, is difficult to explain. It is possible, however, that individuals who are self- selected into an occupation which requires them to deal with high threat situations on a daily basis may differ from the general popu- lation on some variable relevant to denial -sensitization. Such a dif- ference if it existed would make it difficult to interpret these indi- viduals' performances in studies organized around the denial-sensiti- zation dimension. 19 Another finding of these studies has been that providing informa- tion regarding an impending threat reduces arousal when the stressor is finally confronted, particularly if that information is of a nature consistent with the individual's approach to dealing with threat related information. Atheoretical Studies of Coping with Naturally Occurring Stressors Additional evidence for the use of denial as a prominent coping strategy has been derived from atheoretical field studies of the ways in which individuals cope with naturally occurring stressful events. However, in contrast to the conclusions drawn from some of the studies cited above concerning the effectiveness of denial as a coping strategy, these investigations seem to highlight the high costs involved in rely- ing on an avoidant style of dealing with stressful situations. Chodoff et al. (1964), in a study of parents whose children were dying of leukemia or other malignant diseases, found that the use of unusually strong denial defenses was related to a distressing reaction to the child's death and to a difficult period of postmortem mourning. Katz, Weiner, Gallagher and Hellman (1970) studied defensive coping in 30 women awaiting breast biopsy for suspected malignancy. They observed that although denial was extremely effective in relieving anxiety, avoiding somatic symptoms and allowing the patient to function adequately during the anticipatory period, those who relied on this strategy also impaired their chances of survival by waiting longest to consult their physician after discovering a lump in their breast. Boyd, Yeager and McMillan (1973) followed a group of 27 male patients who had major reconstructive surgery for occlusive disease. These patients were observed from the period immediately prior to surgery through the 20 first postsurgical year. Based on the criterion of how soon he re- turned to work or resumed a fulfilling life, the subject was assigned to either a good or poor adjustment group. There were no differences in seriousness of surgery nor in postsurgical complications for the two groups. Based on indepth interviews and a battery of projective and objective questionnaires, the poor adjusters relied on denial to dim their awareness of or to avoid the impending threat. They perceived their doctors as omnipotent figures and took a childlike, passive atti- tude in their relationships with them. These patients blindly accepted the information their doctors provided, seldom sought additional infor- mation or clarification and avoided direct confrontation. When asked about the worst thing that had ever happened to them and how they dealt with it, they gave passive responses and evidenced reliance on magical thinking. Another study which points up the ineffectiveness of denial as a primary strategy was reported by Teichman (1975). He used as his sub- jects 20 Israeli families who had a father, son or husband missing in action during the October 1973 Middle East war. Three coping styles were observed: 1) resignation—a negation of all hope of seeing the family member again, accompanied by engagement in bereavement behaviors; 2) extreme optimi sm--expression of extreme hope accompanied by seeking and accumulating selective information which strengthened their con- victions; and 3) search for objective information and initiation of contact with persons in the same situation. Both resignation and ex- treme optimism were considered defensive strategies because they in- volved denial of reality and because both styles were related to ex- treme crisis when the family member's death was confirmed. 21 Jam's (1965) observed that surgical patients who used denial remained emotionally calm during the period when they were able to deny the possibility of danger but reacted with intense fear or anger when the inescapable signs of danger or suffering were actually en- countered. He explained the mechanism by which denial interferes with effective coping as follows: The arousal of anticipatory fear prior to exposure to a stressful life situation is one of the necessary condi- tions for developing effective inner defenses that enable the person to cope psychologically with stressful stimuli. The effectiveness of the inner defenses which are erected depends upon the degree to which the person can overcome the powerful spontaneous tendency to deny the possibility of being affected by an impending source of danger. (Janis, 1965, p. 227) He called the anticipatory working through or preparing for an impending stressor the "work of worrying" which, like the work of mourning, en- ables a person to cope more effectively in the long run with the stress- ful experience. Evidence for the adaptive value of anticipatory preparation moti- vated by arousal prior to a stressful event of quite a different nature was reported in a study of parachuters (Fenz, 1975). Skillful experi- enced jumpers showed an inverted V-shaped pattern of arousal and avoid- ance responses as measured by respiration, heart rate, GSR and re- sponses to a word association test and a Thematic Apperception Test (TAT) with pictures of varying relevance to parachuting. The peak of arousal for these parachuters occurred in the presence of early, low relevance cues and subsided to a normal level prior to the jump. This pattern is judged to be adaptive in that good performance is highly correlated with a low level of arousal at the time of the jump. 22 Evidence for the Adaptive Value of Strategies Which Incorporate Denial While undoubtedly denial of stimuli which alert one to the threatening aspects of an impending stressful situation can inter- fere with adaptive coping, the use of denial or other defensive strat- egies can at times be functional for the individual. Such a posi- tion is taken by Mechanic (1962), who did a longitudinal study of students coping with Ph.D. qualifying examinations. He followed 20 doctoral level students for a 4 month period beginning 3 months be- fore the examinations and ending approximately 1 month after they were completed, when the results were announced. He found that those students who studied most effectively were those who were able to control their anxiety. They coped by actively seeking information about the exams, organizing their time efficiently and studying with good concentration until anxiety rose to a high enough level to inter- fere with active coping. At this point they employed a variety of intrapsychic and interpersonal defensive maneuvers such as avoidance of anxiety provoking stimuli, magical thinking, humor and external- ization of responsibility for the outcome of the examinations. These defensive strategies helped to contain anxiety and insulate against perceived threat, thus allowing continued coping. While recognizing the dangers of excessive use of defensive strategies which might lead one to restrict oneself to the point where useful coping alter- natives are avoided, Mechanic adopted the position that "adequate adaptation depends on some careful balance between coping and defen- sive processes" (1962, p. 211). 23 Similar views were expressed by Coleman (1973) and Lazarus (in press) both of whom recognized the complexity of coping and the interdependence between defensive strategies such as denial and action-oriented approaches. Coleman believed that the use of de- fenses such as denial, rationalization and emotional isolation fa- cilitated the capacity to deal with stressful situations by defending against intense and disabling anxiety. Lazarus stressed that rather than equate the use of denial with pathology, as has often been done in the past, a more appropriate conclusion would be that mental health requires some self-deception. Strategies Which Combine Denial and Action-Oriented Behaviors That the use of a combined strategy which includes both denial and reality-oriented behavior represents the way that individuals cope with stressful circumstances has been confirmed in several studies in addition to the one by Mechanic (1962) described above. In an investigation of women undergoing mastectomy because of malig- nant breast tumor, Penman (1980) divided 27 patients into those who scored above and below the group mean on a coping adequacy rating based on three dimensions of coping. While the best copers exhib- ited a strategy which relied more on active engagement with issues raised by their diagnosis and surgery than those who coped less ade- quately, they also used a variety of responses which implied evasion of the meaning of their illness, including denial and rationalization. However, they used these strategies to a much lesser extent than they used action-oriented reality responses and with less frequency than poor copers did. 24 Sanders and Kardinal (1977) observed a similar pattern in an in- depth study of four male and two female leukemia patients whose disease was in clinical remission. In addition to reality-based strat- egies which included information seeking, identification with other patients and anticipatory grieving, these subjects used denial of being sick during periods away from the hospital. This use of denial was regarded as adaptive in that it countered the family's insistence on keeping the patient in a sick role and permitted a return to a more normal lifestyle during a time when he/she was feeling well. However, the use of denial in this situation had the potential for becoming problematic if it permitted patients to ignore signs of recur- rence of their disease or interfered with reporting on schedule for maintenance treatment. Another situation in which the use of denial can be adaptive is when a threatening event occurs suddenly, without warning or time for preparation. In such a case preparation time may be bought through a temporary period of self-deception which prevents the individual from being overwhelmed (Hamburg & Adams, 1967). The operation of this mechanism was observed by Hamburg et al . (1953) in their study of severely burned patients and by Visotsky et al . (1961) with individu- als stricken with pol iomelytis. In most patients this defensive re- sponse was noted to gradually give way to a period of active coping during which the seriousness and probable consequences of the circum- stances were realistically faced. Studies Which Emphasize the Effectiveness of Action-Oriented Strategies While coping approaches which combine reality-based action responses with defensive ones have been shown to function well for 25 individuals in a variety of stressful situations, strategies which rely predominately on the former have often been associated with the most positive outcomes. An action-oriented or instrumental strategy is rational, constructive and typically takes the form of problem- solving and decision making (Coleman, 1973). The response may be one which acts upon the stressor to eliminate it, one which acts upon the individual to prepare against harm or to withdraw when threatened by an agent which is overwhelmingly powerful and dangerous (Lazarus, 1977). Evidence that individuals who cope effectively with a number of different types of stressors most often adopt instrumental strategies has been obtained in a number of field studies. Hamburg and Adams (1967) followed a group of highly competent high school seniors through their first year of college to observe how they coped with leaving home and adjusting to college life. Two major approaches, both of which fell under the rubric of instrumental coping, emerged. These students actively sought information about the new situation, the roles they would be required to assume and the difficulties they might encounter. They also adopted a positive attitude toward their new peers and obtained support as well as information from them. Another study of coping techniques which used college students as subjects was reported by Tanck and Robbins (1979). Using a questionnaire they assessed the responses of 83 male and 50 female undergraduates to the stressors of college life and found that the three most frequently reported behaviors were: 1) try to analyze the problem, 2) take direct action to deal with its source and 3) talk it over with friends or family. Each of these responses represented an 26 instrumental strategy which attempted to deal directly with the threatening event rather than escaping or avoiding it. Further support for the adaptive value of instrumental coping was provided by Deaton, Berg, Richlin and Litrownik (1977) who studied the coping behavior of 111 naval officers and aviators who were repatriated prisoners of war (POWs). A questionnaire of behaviors engaged in during captivity was developed based on information ob- tained from other POWs and subjects rated each activity according to its effectiveness in dealing with solitary confinement. This group reported a strong tendency to cope by devoting efforts toward control- ling, changing or mastering the environment rather than by relying on fantasy or other avoidant strategies. They rated activities related to the captor-captive relationship as most useful in coping with the situation. These included attempts to learn about their captors through observation, to anticipate their captors' next move and to develop contingencies to meet new situations as they arose. Repetitive behaviors such as talking to oneself, pacing, watching insects, ritu- alistic acts and withdrawal were judged to be least effective for them. The fact that these men were military officers trained to re- spond under conditions of war may have influenced their choice of strategies or their reporting of them. Coping in a similar circumstance was studied by Ford (1975) who collected data on the 82-man surviving crew of the USS Pueblo, an intelligence vessel captured and held for 11 months in North Korea. Information concerning coping was derived from interview, question- naire and subjects' reports concerning how they and other crew mem- bers fared in captivity. This information was used as the basis for 27 designating crew members as successful or unsuccessful copers. Suc- cessful coping consisted of the ability to defend against excessive anxiety and depression, to contribute to group support and morale and to provide realistic resistance to the captors' demands. Unsuccessful coping was defined by the presence of psychiatric symptoms, behavior detrimental to group morale and/or cooperation with captors as re- ported by peers. The successful copers were found to be more flexible, to make greater use of reality testing and to rely on a greater number of mechanisms than the less successful copers. Cowie (1976) observed the adjustment process of 27 cardiac patients after a sudden, first heart attack. A major element of their coping strategy, as determined from interviews with the patients and their spouses, involved seeking information about heart disease from medical personnel and other patients and using it to reconstruct their past history so as to make sense out of their coronary. This process which involved an instrumental strategy was found to be adaptive in that it took the surprise element out of the event and made it easier to understand and accept. One final piece of evidence which pointed up the adaptiveness of action-oriented coping was obtained in an investigation of relapsed and nonrelapsed alcoholics conducted by Rosenberg (1980). Those who continued to abstain from alcohol tended to deal with stressful events in an active, assertive manner, while those who were drinking again were found to respond to similar events with passivity and compl iance. 28 Summary of Studies of Coping with Naturally Occurring Events From this large group of investigations of how people deal with a variety of stressful circumstances a model of coping can be con- structed. Effective coping seems to depend on the use of instrumental, reality-based responses in anticipating and confronting stressful events. To be capable of making such responses, anxiety must be kept at a manageable level. Defensive strategies which allow the coper to temporarily escape from the demands of the situation can be useful in this regard. Healthy coping appears to be based on use of a variety of responses rather than being limited to one or two and favors a style which emphasizes instrumental responses over passive or avoidant ones. As has been shown, there is great diversity in the ways that individuals respond to stressful situations. Some primarily utilize instrumental strategies, others favor intrapsychic means of avoiding or escaping the stressor and many combine these strategies, relying on one or the other mechanism to varying degrees. An additional com- ponent of coping which has been overlooked in most of these studies was included by Lazarus (1977) in his multidimensional model of coping. These strategies, which he called somatic-oriented behaviors, are ways of reducing the affective, visceral or motor disturbances which often accompany exposure to stressful events. They are similar to defensive strategies in that they do not have an effect on the stressor but rather operate on the person to alter subjective experi- ence. While defensive strategies deny the existence of the event or its significance for the individual, somatic-oriented strategies operate to relieve the physiological distress associated with 29 responding to stressful events without denying or distorting the situation. Somatic discomfort may be manifested as disturbances in sleep pattern, appetite, concentration or mood state. Somatic- oriented strategies allow the person a temporary respite from these distressing accompaniments which interfere with adaptive functioning. Such strategies can improve coping through alleviating somatic symp- toms so that the individual is able to confront situations which might otherwise be avoided or escaped from. Somatic-oriented re- sponses can be divided into those which are functional in increasing the individual's tolerance for or ability to deal with current or future stressors and those which, although capable of temporarily relieving the distressing accompaniments of the situation, also have the potential for becoming additional sources of stress themselves. Examples of functional somatic-oriented behaviors include engaging in vigorous physical exercise, practicing relaxation techniques, listen- ing to music or pursuing a hobby such as gardening, sewing or wood- working. Potentially dysfunctional somatic-oriented behaviors include drinking alcohol, smoking cigarettes, taking tranquilizers or taking sleeping medications. While all of the latter group have the poten- tial for becoming problems in and of themselves, Lazarus (1980) sug- gested that judgment of them as dysfunctional should be reserved until outcomes have been observed for the individuals using them. Determinants of Coping Behavior What are the factors which determine the way in which an indi- vidual responds to a potentially threatening situation? Selye (1976) writes, "in our life events, the stressor effects depend not so much upon what we do or what happens to us but on the way we take it" 30 (p. 370). It is "how you take it" that ultimately determines whether or not one adapts successfully to change. Whether an event will be perceived as threatening depends to a large extent on the interpre- tation made concerning its personal significance and the individual's assessment of his/her ability to deal with it (Lazarus, 1964). This means that the same stimulus can be regarded as threatening or benign depending on the person's appraisal of it. In the past it was fre- quently assumed that the severity of a stress reaction was directly proportional to the intensity of the stressor (Katz et al . , 1970). Yet, there is ample evidence that this is not the case and that it is the individual's subjective judgement of the event rather than any objective criterion which determines its threat value and influences the response that one ultimately makes. Anderson (1976) studied the responses of 93 male owners and managers whose businesses were damaged by flooding after Hurricane Agnes and found that there was a correlation between perceived se- verity of loss (the stressor) as measured by questionnaire, interview and coping response, but that there was no relationship between actual loss based on objective financial data and coping style. Those who perceived their losses as highest, not those who actually suffered the greatest losses, had more defensive, emotional reactions to the situ- ation and engaged in fewer instrumental behaviors directed toward returning the business to its preflood level of performance. Similar results were reported by Friedrich (1979) who investi- gated predictors of coping behavior in mothers of handicapped children. Severity of the handicap was not found to predict coping behavior nor was it found to be positively related to reported stress. Katz et al. 31 (1970) found that women awaiting breast biopsy for suspected breast cancer exhibited a variety of levels of affective distress and of functioning although they were all anticipating the same possibly life-threatening event. Each of these studies which dealt with response to stressful events as a function of perceived severity found that high perceived threat was related to heavy reliance on defensive strategies. This is consonant with Menninger's (1954) statement that minor stressors are usually handled by relatively normal, healthy devices while those experiences perceived as more severe "excite the ego to increasingly energetic and expensive activity" (p. 412) in order to maintain a tolerable level of tension. In recent years researchers have attempted to identify those factors which influence the individual's perception of an event's threat and the tendency to respond either with a reality-based strate- gy or an avoidant, defensive one. Two demographic variables, sex and socioeconomic status (SES), have repeatedly been observed to operate in this process. Sex Differences in Coping Strategy Although researchers have typically not focused on differences in coping strategies utilized by males and females, many who have analyzed their data separately for males and females have found that these groups differ. In an investigation of the way that adults cope with the role stressors of marriage, parenting, family economics and work, Pearlin and Schooler (1978) interviewed a sample of 2,300 adults representative of the population of Chicago. The criterion adopted for effectiveness of a given mechanism was its ability to prevent the 32 experience of distress in the presence of life strains. Distress was measured for each role area by an adjective checklist and strain was assessed in a structured interview. Males were observed more often to employ responses which inhibited stressful outcomes while females more often used mechanisms which resulted in increased distress. Calhoun (1980) also found a difference in the way that males and females coped with marital role strains. A random sample of 434 married graduate students and nonstudent spouses completed a series of questionnaires which identified major areas of marital role strains and types of coping responses. Effectiveness of a style was deter- mined, as in the Pearlin and Schooler (1978) study, by its association with a reduction between strains and perceived stressfulness. Al- though no differences were found in the effectiveness of strategies used by males and females, females reported using more responses di- rected toward trying to change the situation or its meaning, while males tended to avoid the problem or to resign themselves to it. Similar results were obtained in a study of persons coping with marital dissolution (Brown, 1976). An assessment of coping strategy was conducted based on two indepth interviews with 429 males and fe- males who contacted a court marriage counseling service. The first interview was done soon after their first contact with the service and the second, 4 months later. Respondents were assigned to one of 11 coping styles based on their behavior during the 4 month interval directed toward attaining goals stated at the first interview and on the subject's perception of how he/she dealt with the divorce situ- ation. Men were found to rely more on denial than women. The per- centage of males classified in one of two avoidance-denial styles 33 was 33% whereas only 5% of females were characterized by these styles. However, women were overrepresented in the three other styles which, along with denial, were associated with increased distress over the period covered by the study (13% of males as compared with 30% of females). Instrumental styles, the most effective strategies in terms of lowering distress, were utilized approximately equally by the two groups although females employed them slightly more frequently than males (52% of females compared to 46% of males). These findings along with those reported by Calhoun (1980) in regard to use of denial are inconsistent with those reported by Pearlin and Schooler (1978) who found that males used more responses indicative of self-reliance and controlled reflectiveness, while females made more responses to selec- tively ignore or deny their problems. In two studies which assessed coping in college students male- female differences were also observed. Tanck and Robbins (1979) ad- ministered a self-report itemized coping style questionnaire to 83 male and 50 female undergraduates. While both groups most frequently chose instrumental responses as representative of the way they handled stressful situations, males were more likely than females to use mari- juana or seek sexual comfort, while females more often than males re- ported that they became irritable and easily angered, spent hours thinking about the problem, ate constantly or ceased functioning effec- tively. Two of the responses reported more frequently by females than by males were associated with measures of anxiety and depression. Neither of the responses favored by males was associated with anxiety or depression. Anxiety and depression were measured by responses to a self-report questionnaire on which subjects were asked to indicate 34 their recent levels of tension and/or depression on a 4-point scale, to estimate the duration of these feelings and to report what they thought was the probable cause. In another study which used college undergraduates as subjects, Thelen and Varble (1970) compared 28 males and 37 females who came for counseling at the campus mental health clinic with 61 male and 51 female introductory psychology students. Neither group had been in personal counseling in the past two years. Coping and defense strate- gies were measured by California Psychological Inventory (CPI) and MMPI items designated by Haan (1964). The client group scored higher than the psychology students on three of the seven defense scales while the psychology students scored higher than the client group on four of the seven coping scales. There were male-female differences in both groups. On the coping scales males in the nonclient group but not females scored higher than the client group on objectivity, while females but not males scored higher on concentration. On the defense scales client-group males scored higher on projection and doubt than nonclient-group males but there were no differences between females of the two groups on these scales. An unusual finding was that nonclient- group males scored higher than client-group males and females on denial . Summary of Male-Female Differences in Coping That there are differences in the strategies employed by males and females seems to be the only truly consistent finding in this area. Both males and females appear to rely more on instrumental strategies than on defensive ones. When a defensive strategy is chosen, males are probably more likely to use denial than females and females are 35 more likely than males to experience increased distress, anxiety and depression, as a result of their choice of a defensive strategy. Those who have noted greater use of instrumental styles by males than by females have attributed these differences to socialization in a culture which emphasizes active mastery of the environment more for males than for females (Brown, 1976; Pearlin & Schooler, 1978). How- ever, from the findings reported here it is unclear that males use more instrumental strategies than females; rather the greatest differences between the two groups appears to be in the choice of defensive re- sponses. SES Differences in Coping Style In addition to sex the other demographic variable which has been associated with coping style in a number of studies is SES. SES is the individual's status in the social system and is generally assessed with reference to education and occupation, which usually determine income and access to material goods and services. The relationship between SES and coping style has been consistent, with membership in higher SES groups positively correlated with effective coping. It has often been asserted that members of lower socioeconomic strata experi- ence more stressful life events and have fewer personal and material resources with which to cope. Their families are more likely to be disrupted by separation, divorce or premature death, thus depriving them of a major source of support in handling stressful life events (Dohrenwend & Dohrenwend, 1967). Because of their lower level of edu- cation, verbal skills which facilitate manipulation of environmental circumstances may be less well-developed than in middle class persons. Evidence that lower SES members are highly vulnerable to breakdown in 36 coping mechanisms has been provided by two epidemiological studies of the relationship between stressful events and depressive symptomology. Brown, Bhrolchain and Harris (1975) in a study of a random sample of 200 women living in London and 114 female outpatients being treated for depression found that working class women with young children were the most likely to experience failure in their coping strategy as evidenced by the presence of clinical depression. This increased vul- nerability was correlated with a high level of ongoing stressful life events and lack of psychological support from a close interpersonal relationship. A similar finding was reported by Warheit (1979) in a two-stage study of 517 persons subsampled from 1,645 statistical probability sample of adults living in the southeastern United States. Stressful life events, coping resources (personal, familial, social and cultural) and depressive symptomology were assessed from data obtained from two interviews conducted 3 years apart, utilizing a 317-question inter- view schedule. Highest depression scores were found in those who ex- perienced high rates of stressful events involving loss and that were either from the lower SES strata or did not have friends nearby to help. Low SES was found to correlate positively with both high loss and fewer personal and social resources with which to cope. Several other investigations have noted SES differences in re- lation to coping effectiveness. Parent (1976) studied stress and coping in 113 male and female adults living in a southern city and found that people from lower SES groups differed from those from higher SES groups in their greater use of indirect strategies. Penman (1980), who followed mastectomy patients for 4 months after their surgery, 37 found that those who made the best adjustment were those who relied predominately on instrumental responses in dealing with the issues raised by their diagnosis and surgery. Although she found no differ- ences between these women and those who used avoidant, passive or fatalistic styles on 12 of 14 demographic and health variables, there were positive relationships between adaptive coping and education and income. Finally, Brown (1976) found that positive psychological out- come during a 4 month period of marital dissolution was positively correlated with education, one measure of social class, but not with family income. Although sex and SES are important variables which often influ- ence the way a potentially stressful situation is appraised and re- sponded to, an even greater influence may be exerted by the individu- al's judgment of the ability to master the situation. Locus of Control as a Mediator of Responses to Stressful Events Many who have studied variations in individual responses to similar stressful situations have been aware of the importance of belief about one's potential for influencing the environment as a determinant of whether an event will be perceived as benign or threat- ening and whether a strategy to change the situation or to attempt to deny or avoid it will be chosen. The person's belief about self in this regard has been mentioned as an important characteristic in inter- preting and responding to stimuli by Dohrenwend and Dohrenwend (1978), Lazarus (1977), Lefcourt (1973), Pearlin and Schooler (1978) and Rab- kin and Struening (1976). The belief concerning personal efficacy has been called locus of control by Rotter (1966) who developed the Internal -External Locus of 38 Control Scale (I-E Scale), an instrument to measure it. The construct of locus of control is based on social learning theory and is consid- ered to be a generalized expectancy which operates across a large number of situations regarding the nature of the causal relationship between one's behavior and its consequences (Lefcourt, 1966).'' When out- come or reinforcement is believed by the individual to be contingent upon personal actions, the person is designated an internal (I) and when reinforcement is attributed to luck, chance, fate or powerful others, the individual is designated an external (E). 'Locus of control may be considered a key determinant of the coping process in that it represents an underlying personality trait which influences perception of personal power vis-a-vis a situation and expectation concerning out- come. Persons who feel powerless to direct their own experiences are unlikely to initiate responses to take direct action to control a stressful event. They are more likely to respond in a manner con- sistent with their belief that little which they do will have an effect (Wolk & Kurtz, 1975). On the other hand, internally controlled per- sons are more apt to be alert to those aspects of the environment which provide information relevant to their future behavior and to take steps to improve environmental conditions (Rotter, 1966). Field Studies Relating Locus of Control to Coping Strategies A number of researchers have studied the relationship of locus of control to choice of coping strategy. Anderson (1977), in his study of male business owners'/managers' responses to flood damage (cited previously), tested the hypothesis that locus of control led to differ- ences in perception of a stressful situation and affected performance. Subjects were given Rotter's I-E Scale and scores were correlated with 39 subjective perception of stressful ness and coping strategy. Perception of stressfulness was measured by the Subjective Stress Scale-Form B, a list of 14 adjectives from which the subject chooses the one which most adequately describes his/her strongest feeling about a situation. Assessment of coping was based on data obtained in a structured inter- view. Coping responses were assigned to one of two categories, prob- lem solving behaviors or emotional reactions. Externals perceived greater stress and employed fewer task-oriented problem solving behav- iors and more defensive behaviors than internals. Task-oriented re- sponses were positively correlated with high levels of organizational performance in returning the business to preflood operational status. Anderson concluded that locus of control influenced performance pri- marily through choice of task versus emotional -oriented coping behavior. Wolk and Kurtz (1975) in a study of 92 older adults between the ages of 60 and 85 assessed the relationship between locus of control and three separate measures of adjustment; a scale based on develop- mental tasks of the later years, an instrument which measured social, psychological and physiological involvement and the Life Satisfaction Index. On all three measures internals manifested higher levels of adjustment. In a study which defined effective coping as continued good physi- cal health despite high levels of stressful life events over a period of two years, Barber (1979) conducted an indepth interview with two groups of employees of a California school district. All had experi- enced crisis levels of stressors as defined by the Holmes and Rahe Social Readjustment Rating Scale (Holmes & Rahe, 1967), but the two groups differed in relation to physical well-being as assessed by a 40 health questionnaire. Subjects were also given the I-E Scale, along with several other personality instruments. Healthier subjects were more internal and accepted more responsibility for what happened in their lives. Dinardo (1972) in a study of adjustment to spinal cord injury found differences in 53 hospitalized males as a function of their scores on the I-E Scale. Internals were less likely than externals to be depressed, a factor shown to favor good adjustment to spinal cord injury. In her study of adjustment to divorce (cited above) Brown (1976) correlated coping style with locus of control for the female subjects of her sample and found that internals most often utilized instrumental styles, while the more external subjects were most often categorized as paralyzed victims because of their failure to make any behavioral attempt to change their situation. Tanck and Robbins (1979) in their questionnaire assessment of coping in male and female college students also observed differences in strategy for externals and internals. Internal females were more likely to take direct action and to meditate, while their external counterparts were more prone to seek sexual comfort or to use alcohol as a way of dealing with the stressors of college life. External males re- ported daydreaming, fantasizing or seeking professional help as repre- sentative of the ways in which they behaved in stressful situations. In a study of the ways in which people coped with depression, Rippere (1976) administered an antidepressive behavior checklist to 40 male and 40 female subjects who comprised five groups. Subjects were depressed outpatients, depressed inpatients, nondepressed outpatients, 41 clinical psychology graduate students and general practice medical patients. Subjects checked the number of behaviors they used when they felt depressed and evaluated each one in regard to its helpful- ness. A version of the I-E Scale modified for use with British re- spondents was used to divide the subjects into internals and externals. No differences were found as a function of group, sex or locus of con- trol. However, there was a significant interaction between sex and locus of control with internal males and external females reporting fewer antidepressive activities and finding fewer of these activities helpful than external males and internal females. No attempt was made to explain the unusual finding that internality in males was related to use of fewer antidepressive behaviors while internality was associated with use of a greater number of antidepressive behaviors in females. Although Pearlin and Schooler's (1978) study of responses to four different types of role strains in the general population (cited pre- viously) did not use the I-E Scale, mastery, which was defined as the extent to which one regards one's life circumstances as being under one's own control versus fatalistically ruled, was measured by an in- strument developed by the researchers. Mastery was found to relate positively to effective coping and to achieved status, expecially edu- cation and income. Horn (1975) investigated the adjustment of 15 high and 15 low- level functioning women to past negative experiences. Level of func- tioning was measured by the Personal Orientation Inventory and by fac- tors of the Edwards Personal Preference Scale. While the two groups did not differ in exposure to stressful negative experiences such as divorce, death, sickness, accident, 42 hostility and violence, their retrospective perceptions of the experi- ences and their personal adjustment in relation to them differed remarkably. High-level functioning women were found to be more inner directed, to have relied more often on self-help rather than profes- sional help in coping with the negative events and were more likely to have accepted and integrated the experiences into their personality than low-level functioning women. Although locus of control was not a variable in this study, the inner directedness of the high-level func- tioning group is similar to the construct of internality. Two other studies which correlated locus of control with coping mechanisms found no significant relationship between the two variables. Chamberlain (1980) studied the responses of 55 oil company executives and 20 spouses concerning stressful events which they experienced over a two week period. Using a questionnaire he obtained information about the source of stress, its perceived magnitude and how it was dealt with. Coping strategy was measured by the Byrne R-S Scale, an instrument which conceptualizes coping along a continuum of vigilance/avoidance in regard to the threatening aspects of a situation. Subjects also were administered the I-E Scale. Locus of control was not found to be a significant factor in determining perceived magnitude of stress nor in choice of coping response. Ritter (1980) also failed to find a relationship between locus of control and coping style for 37 male and 13 female patients awaiting open-heart surgery. Patients were inter- viewed before and after receiving preoperative information on the day before their surgery. As in the Chamberlain study, coping was assessed on the vigilance/avoidance continuum and locus of control was measured by the I-E Scale. 43 Experimental Studies Relating Response to Aversive Events to Locus of Control Several studies conducted in the laboratory have investigated the relationship between locus of control and coping with stressful situations. Two such studies conducted at a Canadian university ob- served differences between internals and externals to experimental manipulations which attempted to deceive and embarrass subjects. Lef- court, Gronnerud and McDonald (1973) exposed 65 male subjects, who had been solicited for participation in a test of verbal facility, to a list of words which was intended to arouse suspicion that the experi- menter had been dishonest in his description of the task and intended to embarrass them. After the first 13 words which were nonsexual in nature, every third, then every second and eventually every word was a sexual double entendre. Tape recordings of response times and ver- bal answers were made and subjects were also videotaped through a one- way mirror so that their nonverbal behavior in response to their dis- covery of the experimenter's duplicity could be observed. The de- pendent variable was the number of humor responses made by the subject as he became aware of the true nature of the experiment. Responding with humor was considered by the researchers indicative of lack of apprehension and as an adaptive response to the situation. Internals as measured by the I-E Scale exhibited more smiles and laughter than externals who tended to respond to the situation with gravity. In a second "double cross" experiment conducted to obtain evidence for the reliability of the results just cited, Sardoni (cited in Lef- court, 1976) showed subjects mug shots of a series of individuals identified as criminals and asked them to identify the type of offense 44 committed by each. During the experiment, without the knowledge of the subject, a picture was taken of him and presented as the tenth criminal. After the presentation of the subject's photograph, three pictures of well-known political figures were shown. The purpose of the experiment was to observe the responses of internal and external subjects to evidence that they had been tricked. Internals more often responded with humor than externals, while externals more often re- sponded with annoyance and self-consciousness. Whether these manipu- lations actually represented threatening situations is questionable, however, consistent differences were observed between externals and internals across the two situation. Another experimental study which observed differences between internals and externals in a stressful situation was reported by Gal (1976). Two hundred male students were divided into Is and Es based on the I-E Scale and exposed to one of three threat condi tions—shock- contingent, shock-noncontingent or no shock. In the shock-contingent condition, delivery of the aversive stimulus was contingent upon the subject's performance on a task, in the noncontingent condition shock was unavoidable and in the no shock condition, shock was not mentioned. According to GSR, skin conductance and heart rate, as well as subjec- tive report, internals were found to be more tense performing under the shock-contingent condition than under the noncontingent condition. The reverse was true for externals. These findings were contrary to the hypotheses of the study which predicted that each group would be more comfortable performing under conditions congruent with their generalized expectancy concerning control of events. 45 Summary of Research Relating Locus of Control to Coping Strategy Although these research findings do not present a totally con- sistent picture of the relationship between locus of control, appraisal of threat and choice of coping strategy, they do support the hy- pothesis that locus of control is an important variable in this regard. Most of the studies cited reported that internals perceived a situ- ation as less stressful, exhibited lower levels of arousal and em- ployed coping strategies which relied predominately on instrumental, problem solving techniques. Externals, on the other hand, were shown to perceive a situation as more stressful, to exhibit higher levels of physiological arousal and to use more defensive coping strategies. Internality has also been shown to correlate with the absence of physical symptoms in the presence of high levels of stressful life events, good adjustment to spinal cord injury, positive adjustment in the later years and more effective coping in a variety of naturally occurring and experimentally contrived circumstances representing threat. Research on Coping in Mental Health Clients Although a great deal of research has been done in the area of coping, very little is known about the process as it operates in those who seek professional help in dealing with stressful life events. The few studies which have focused on counseling clients have either been limited in scope and have overlooked the complexity of the process or have utilized very small samples. Five studies were included in the literature; two of these were conducted with student samples and three used as subjects adults seeking counseling services in community facil- ities. 46 Coelho, Silber and Hamburg (1962) assessed coping in three groups of college freshmen by means of a projective test, the Student TAT, which was developed for this purpose. Subjects included 30 male and female freshmen selected from three groups: 10 highly competent students chosen for their social, academic and interpersonal adjust- ment, 10 students from an English class at a state university and 10 students who were hospitalized in a psychiatric ward because of emo- tional difficulties experienced during their freshman year. Subjects were shown a set of slides which represented college situations which tended to be new and problematic for entering freshmen and were in- structed to describe the situation which led up to the scene, what was happening at the present time, the thoughts and feelings of the charac- ters and what the outcome would be. A coping index was developed based on the number and type of activities attributed to the hero and the favorableness of outcome. Although no significant differences were found between the two normal groups, there were differences between normals and patients with greatest differences between the patient group and the highly competent student group. Patients showed lower problem solving competence, found fewer solutions to the potentially stressful situations and projected favorable outcomes in only one-half of the situations. The normal groups coped through active effort and were optimistic about outcomes compared with the student patients who coped passively and with pessimism. Another study which included a client group was reported by Thelen and Varble (1970) (cited above), who compared coping styles for 28 male and 37 female college students who came for treatment at the university ntal health clinic and 61 male and 51 female introductory psychology me 47 students. Items from the CPI and MMPI as designated by Haan (1964) were used as measures of coping and defense. Differences between the treatment group and the nontreatment group were found on the coping scales and on the defense scales. The nontreatment group scored higher on three of the four coping scales for which differences were observed and the treatment group scored higher on three of the four defense scales where differences were found. Both of these studies were conducted with student populations. The former included only 10 patients and did not analyze the data separately for males and females. The latter included a larger sample and conducted separate analyses by sex. However, neither study con- trolled for SES nor locus of control, two variables consistently shown to be relevant to the coping process. Three studies have appeared in the literature which focused on adults who sought mental health services at community facilities. Overbeck (1977) did an exploratory investigation of stressful ante- cedents to application for help at a community clinic and obtained information about the responses clients made to these events. Only three adult outpatients were interviewed along with 16 parents of children being treated in the children's unit and one adolescent client. This small sample was all female except for one father. The Social Readjustment Rating Scale (Holmes & Rahe, 1967) was adminis- tered to assess stressful life events and an indepth interview was conducted to obtain information about a number of client variables in- cluding response to stressful events. Coping strategies were divided into four broad categories: cognitive, affective, behavioral and somatic. No data were presented, however the following statement was made with regard to coping: Most subjects expressed inability to cope effectively with their circumstances: several clearly found their style of coping inadequate; e.g., 'doing something' (behavior coping) did not help--there was a need to know (cognitive coping) what the trouble was; or 'I knew what was wrong but I couldn't decide how I felt about it' (affective coping); or 'everytime he did that, I would end up in bed (somatic coping) and that didn't help things'. Subjects tended to repeat compulsively their coping styles even when they were highly ineffective. (Overbeck, 1977, p. 218) Rippere (1976) (cited previously) also conducted a study of coping mechanisms which included a small group of outpatients. Data on how individuals dealt with depression were collected by means of a check- list of antidepressive behaviors. Clients drawn from depressed out- patients, depressed inpatients, nondepressed inpatients, clinical psychology graduate students and general practice medical patients formed five groups of 16 each. Data were analyzed separately for males and females and for internals and externals. The only results pre- sented concerned the number of antidepressive activities engaged in and the number of these found by the individual to be helpful. De- pressed outpatients were not found to differ from the other groups on either of these variables. However, a significant interaction was found between locus of control and sex. Internal males and external females reported fewer responses and found less of them helpful than internal females and external males. Whether depression is actually a stressor which must be coped with is a debatable issue in itself. It has been suggested that depression represents a response to failure to cope effectively with stressful events, yet many individuals find the concomitants of depression dysphoric and initiate behaviors directed toward alleviating them. 49 A final study of this type was done by Brown (1976) (also cited previously). The coping behavior of 198 male and 270 female appli- cants to a court marriage counseling service was assessed by two in- depth interviews conducted soon after the first contact with the agency and again 4 months later. Responses to the stressor of marital dis- solution and behaviors engaged in aimed at the attainment of goals stated during the first interview were assigned to one of 11 coping style categories. Results were analyzed separately for males and females. Locus of control was assessed for females and correlated with coping strategy but was not measured for the male subjects. Internal females were found to experience greater reduction of dis- tress between the two interviews, while externals more often em- ployed strategies which were associated with continued distress. Education and income were tested for their effect on coping for females but not for males. Coping styles differed significantly according to years of education but not according to family income. Education was positively related to effective coping with instru- mental copers having a significantly higher mean level of education than those who used defensive or passive styles. Although this study did take into account sex, SES and locus of control as they related to the coping process, the primary focus was on how females coped with marital dissolution and for that reason data were not presented for males on SES or locus of control. A major shortcoming of this study was that although data were collected at two stages with clients who applied for counseling, no attempt was made to account for the effect of the counseling process on type of coping strategy used or on change in level of distress. It is likely that some clients never 50 returned for a second appointment while others may have participated in counseling for the entire 4 month period covered by the study. As can be seen, more research is needed with adult clinical outpatients to fully understand how the coping process operates within this group. An adequate study must include a large represen- tative sample and control for the relevant variables of sex, SES and locus of control. Additionally, such an investigation should use a measure of coping strategies which reflects the complexity of the coping process. The present study was designed to meet these cri- teria. The focus was on identifying the relationship between locus of control and coping strategies in male and female community mental health outpatients. Assessment of coping was conducted by means of an inventory developed by the researcher, based on the theoretical position of Lazarus (1977). Coping response categories included direct action, defensive, functional somatic-oriented and potentially dysfunctional somatic-oriented strategies. The following hypotheses were tested: Null Hypotheses HO There is no positive linear relationship between inter- nality and choice of direct action and functional somatic- oriented responses. HO There is no negative linear relationship between inter- 1 B nality and choice of defensive and potentially dysfunc- tional somatic-oriented responses. HO There is no negative linear relationship between exter- L A nality and choice of direct action and functional somatic- oriented responses. 51 H0OD There is no positive linear relationship between exter- 2B nality and choice of defensive and potentially dysfunc- tional somatic-oriented responses. CHAPTER III METHOD Subjects Individuals who requested counseling services through the Out- patient Department of the Palm Beach County Comprehensive Community Mental Health Center, who were nonpsychotic and who had not received counseling during the preceding six months, served as subjects. Data collection was initiated December 1, 1981, and continued through June 15, 1982, when 100 questionnaires had been completed. The demographic characteristics of the research subjects are shown in Table 1. There were no statistically significant differences in sex, race and education between these subjects and the population which is served by the Center's Outpatient Department, based on data avail- able for the previous year beginning August 1, 1980, and ending July 31, 1981. However, a significantly larger proportion of the research sub- jects were younger than 30 years of age. There were also some differ- ences between the research subjects and the outpatient population on marital status. A significantly greater proportion of the research sub- jects had never married and a lesser proportion were presently married. There were no differences between the sample and the Center's out- patient population in the proportion of separated/divorced and widowed subjects. These subjects are similar to client populations served by many community mental health outpatient clinics in urban areas throughout the country with the exception of those which serve high percentages of minority group members. 52 53 Table 1 Demographic Characteristics of Research Subjects Group Group Sex Male Female Aqe < 30 30-49 50-65 > 65 Race White Black Hispanic Marital status Single Married Divorced Widowed 40 60 51 37 08 04 92 06 02 37 32 28 03 Years of education < 12 20 12-15 66 > 16 14 Social class I 02 II 05 III 25 IV 52 V 16 Type of problem Relationship 26 Depression 16 Anxiety 04 Combination of above 45 Other 09 Referral source Self 45 Family/friend 33 Agency 16 Physician 06 54 Instruments Adult Nowicki-Strickland Internal-External Control Scale The Adult Nowicki-Strickland Internal-External Control Scale (ANS-IE) (See Appendix A) was used as a measure of locus of control, an individual's tendency to perceive reinforcement as being under one's own control as opposed to attributing control to chance, fate or powerful others. The ANS-IE was developed in response to the need for a scale to overcome the problems attributed to the Rotter Locus of Control Scale (Nowicki, 1979). The Rotter scale has shown a con- sistent relationship to social desirability responses and to denial of psychopathology. Rotter items have also been criticized for con- founding personal, social, political and ideological causation. Additionally, the difficult reading level of the scale has made it inappropriate for a large segment of the population. The ANS-IE was constructed on the basis of Rotter's definition of the internal -external control of reinforcement dimension and contains items which describe reinforcement situations across the interpersonal and motivational areas of affiliation, achievement and dependency (Nowicki & Strickland, 1973). The scale is a paper and pencil measure consisting of 40 questions which are answered "Yes" or "No" by placing a mark in the appropriate blank next to each item. Items are written at the fifth grade reading level. Scores range from 0 through 40 with higher scores reflecting greater externality. The scale has been administered to a variety of groups including male and female college students, community subjects, racial and ethnic groups and subjects with physical and emotional disorders. 55 Norms are available for each of these groups (Nowicki , 1979). The psychometric characteristics of the ANS-IE are reported below. Item statistics. Item-total score correlations for a college sample (in = 154) and a sample of community adults (n = 33) ranged from .01 to .60 (Nowicki, 1979). Internal consistency. Nowicki and Duke (1974) reported split-half reliabilities ranging from .74 to .86 based on a series of studies. Because the items are not arranged in order according to difficulty, the split-half reliabilities represent an underestimate of the true internal consistency reliability. Test-retest reliability. Test-retest reliability for college students over a six weeks period was reported to be .83 (Nowicki & Duke, 1974) and .65 for a seven week period (Chandler, 1976). Discriminative validity. ANS-IE scores were not found to be re- lated to social desirability as measured by the Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale for two samples of college students (n = 48, r = .10; n = 68, r = .06). The relationship of ANS-IE scores to intelli- gence, as measured by scores on the Scholastic Aptitude Test, was also investigated and found not to be significant (n = 48, _r = .11). Construct validity. The ANS-IE and the Rotter Locus of Control Scale were administered to two college samples and one community adult sample (r = .68, df = 47, £ < .01 ; r = .48, df = 37, £<.01; r = .44, df = 33, £<.05). These results are consistent with the contention that the two measures are assessing the same construct but not in an identical manner. The ANS-IE was also found to relate significantly to Levinson's Internal ity (r = -.24, n = 1 1 95, £< .01 ) ; Powerful Others (r = .24, n = 1196, £< .01 ) and Chance (r = .40, n - 1195, £ < .01) scales. 56 Additional evidence for the validity of the ANS-IE as a measure of locus of control was provided by the following findings which were predicted by social learning theory upon which the locus of control construct is based. Internal ity, as measured by the ANS-IE, was found by Mink (cited in Nowicki, 1979) to correlate positively with higher family income, educational and occupational levels. Blacks tended to be more external than whites even when middle class black college students were compared with middle class white students. These findings are consistent with the expectation that individuals who are restricted by social and environmental barriers and who are sub- jected to limited access to goods and services would develop an exter- nally-oriented outlook on life (Joe, 1971). In the area of psycho- pathology, social learning theory predicts and most investigators, whether they have used the Rotter scale, the ANS-IE or other measures of control orientation, have found that externality is related to psychological maladjustment (Brannigan, Rosenberg & Loprete, 1977; Duke & Mullens, 1973; Joe, 1971). Significant differences on the ANS-IE were observed between hospitalized schizophrenics (mean = 16.30), hospitalized nonschizophrenics (mean - 11.95) and staff workers (mean = 9.20) (Nowicki & Duke, 1974). High externality scores were also cor- related with high Neuroticism scores on the Eysenck scale (males, r = .36, £ < .10; females,r = .40, £<.05) (Nowicki , 1979). Coping Response Inventory The Coping Response Inventory (CRI) (See Appendix A), a self- report of coping behaviors developed by the researcher, was used as a measure of behaviors engaged in as a response to events or situa- tions which subjects defined as stressful. The CRI is a pencil and 57 paper measure consisting of 24 items which are rated on a 7-point Likert-type scale. The scale was developed based upon Lazarus' (1977) formulation of the coping process and includes categories of responses which he defined as direct action, defensive and somatic-oriented. The somatic-oriented category was divided by the researcher into two cate- gories which have been designated as functional somatic-oriented and potentially dysfunctional somatic-oriented. Direct action responses are efforts which are aimed directly at influencing or eliminating the harmful nature of the stressor. De- fensive responses are attempts to deny or ameliorate one's perception of a threatening situation so that threat is reduced. Threat, however, is reduced only in the mind of the individual as no action is taken to influence the stressor. Somatic-oriented responses are efforts to reduce the dysphoric somatic conditions which accompany the process of anticipating and responding to stressful events. Somatic-oriented responses are directed toward alleviating somatic manifestations such as anxiety, loss of appetite, sleeplessness, impaired concentration or depressed mood state. Functional somatic-oriented responses are those which relieve somatic dysphoria and increase one's capacity for coping. Potentially dysfunctional somatic-oriented responses are those which relieve somatic discomfort but which have the potential for be- coming additional sources of stress themselves. Eight items which were judged by the researcher to be behavioral examples of these coping response categories were drawn, with per- mission of the authors, from a coping response questionnaire developed by Tanck and Robbins (1979). For example, "Try analyzing the problem" and "Talk the problem over with friends or family" were selected as 58 responses which represented direct action strategies. "Daydream or fantasize" was chosen as an example of a defensive strategy and "En- gage in vigorous exercise" was selected as an example of a functional somatic-oriented response. "Take tranqui 1 izing medicines" and "Drink alcohol" were chosen to represent potentially dysfunctional somatic- oriented responses. Listed below are the items as they appeared on the Tanck and Robbins scale: Try analyzing the problem. Talk the problem over with friends or family. Daydream or fantasize. Engage in vigorous exercise. Watch TV or go to a movie. Eat constantly. Drink alcohol . Take tranquil izing medicines. Two of these items were modified to avoid negative connotations. The word "fantasize" was dropped from the item "Daydream or fantasize" and the phrase "think about more pleasant things" was substituted. The word "constantly" was omitted from the item "Eat constantly" and the item was rewritten as "Eat more." An additional 16 items were written by the researcher with the assistance of a member of the University of Florida Student Mental Health clinical staff. Each item represented an attempt to opera- tionally define the four categories of responses specified by Lazarus (1977). These items are listed below: Find out everything I can about the situation. Read about how others have solved similar problems. 59 Consider all the alternatives. Try to make a decision. Focus on the concerns of others instead of my problem. Decide that the problem doesn't really affect me. Believe that things will take care of themselves. Ignore the problem. Try to relax or rest. Read or listen to music. Garden or work in the yard. Take a hot bath or shower. Do some work with my hands (sew, paint, do carpentry). Sleep a lot. Smoke more. Take sleeping medicines. These 16 items were combined with the eight items from the Tanck and Robbins scale to form the CRI. A pilot study of the CRI was conducted to establish the relia- bility and validity of the instrument. The questionnaire was adminis- tered to outpatient clients of the Palm Beach County Comprehensive Community Mental Health Center (_n = 50) and to nonclient community subjects (n - 102). The psychometric properties of the CRI, which are based upon this pilot study are presented below. Test-retest reliability. Test-retest reliability coefficients for the four subscales for nonclient adults (n = 34) over a 6-week period were direct action, .90; defensive, .70; functional somatic- oriented, .80; and potentially dysfunctional somatic-oriented, .80. Coefficients for a group of community mental health clients [n = 33) 60 during ongoing therapy for a 6-8 week period were direct action, .65; defensive, .70; functional somatic-oriented, .60; and potentially dysfunctional somatic-oriented, .75. These lower correlations are consistent with the expectation that coping behavior is affected by the therapy process. These reliability coefficients are based on responses to the CRI in the format of a 4-point scale with categories of "Never," Some- times," "Usually" and "Always." Evidence that some subjects consid- ered the four categories inadequate to describe their behavior was suggested by check marks placed on the lines which divided two cate- gories and by responses such as "Almost Never" or "Almost Always" written on the questionnaire. In response to this feedback and in an effort to increase reliability the scale was changed from a 4 to a 7-point scale before being administered to the dissertation research subjects. "Rating scales, like test scores, tend to increase in re- liability as the number of ratable categories and items increases" (Sax, 1974, p. 482). Content validity. A list of the items grouped together according to category was sent to two groups working in the field of stress re- search, Dr. Richard Lazarus, Director of the Stress and Coping Project, University of California, Berkeley and Drs. Tanck and Robbins of George Washington University. Their comments as to the appropriateness of the items were requested. Feedback was elicited from these two research groups because they were engaged in research which was closely related to the topic of the present research. Dr. Lazarus was the author of the theory of coping upon which the present study is based and Drs. Tanck and Robbins (1979) published a 22-item coping response 61 questionnaire which they used in a study of asserti veness , locus of control and coping behaviors in college students. Eight items from the Tanck and Robbins questionnaire were included in the CRI. Dr. Lazarus offered the following comments: I am glad you sent me your coping materials. You are obviously thinking along lines similar in many ways to those of my own research group. My only important criticism has to do with the prior judgment that some modes of somatic-oriented coping are inherently dys- functional, or for that matter, functional. This indeed could be the case, but I think the decision depends on the outcomes, and perhaps on the pattern- ing of coping employed by individuals. (Lazarus, 1980, p. 1) In response to this feedback, the category dysfunctional somatic- oriented responses was modified by adding the word "potentially." Dr. Robbins responded to the request sent to the Tanck and Robbins research team. He wrote: In general, your items seem to fit within the cate- gories you are using .... I have no problems with categories 1 , 3 & 4. The items seem O.K. Category 2 concerns me somewhat because it deals at least in part with unconscious processes as described by Lazarus (projection, e.g.,) and yet the items tend to be conscious decisions .... I like the idea of a coping inventory and I suggest a factor analysis of your items might be useful to see if they cluster along lines indicated by Lazarus. (Robbins, 1980, p. 1 ) Construct validity. In response to Dr. Robbins' suggestion a principal-component factor analysis with varimax rotation was con- ducted on the responses of 143 pilot study subjects. This analysis verified the existence of the four coping response categories. Four factors with eigenvalues greater than 1 accounted for 75% of the total variance. Table 2 summarizes the results of the varimax rotated factor matrix. 62 Table 2 Pilot Study Varimax Rotated Factor Matrix Item Item Loading Direct action factor Talk the problem over with a friend .33 or family member. Try analyzing the problem. .71 Find out everything I can about the situation. .77 Consider all the alternatives. .70 Try to make a decision. -75 Defensive factor Sleep a lot. -40 Believe that things will take care of themselves. .33 Ignore the problem. -53 Get away from the problem by watching T.V. .36 Eat more. -33 Daydream or think about more pleasant things. .67 Functional somatic-oriented factor Exercise--take a walk, jog, swim, dance. .33 Garden or work in the yard. -77 Do some work with my hands (sew, paint, do carpentry, etc.). -57 Potentially dysfunctional somatic-oriented factor Take tranquil izing medicines. -94 Take sleeping medicines. -71 Note: Table includes factors with eigenvalues greater than 1 63 As can be seen, items from the direct action and defensive cate- gories clustered together as expected, providing strong evidence for the existence of these two response categories. Limited validity was provided for the two somatic-oriented categories. Only two of the six items defined by the researcher as potentially dysfunctional somatic-oriented responses clustered together. A third item from this category, "Drink Alcohol" correlated .25 with this factor. The items "Sleep a Lot" and "Eat More" clustered with items defining the defensive category. In the functional somatic-oriented category only three of six items correlated .33 or greater with each other. A fifth factor with an eigenvalue of .92 contained the remaining three items from this category. A second factor analysis which combined the pilot study data with the dissertation research data was conducted and the results of this second analysis were used in selecting the items for testing the hypotheses of the study. Results of this analysis are presented in the Data Analysis section. Demographic Data Form A demographic data form (See Appendix B) was used to obtain infor- mation concerning client age, sex, race, marital status, education, occupation, presenting problem and referral source. This information was used to provide a description of the demographic characteristics of the sample and in the assignment of SES scores. Determination of SES was based on the Hollingshead Two Factor Index of Social Position (Hollingshead, 1957). Occupation and education are the two factors which are used by the Index in determining SES. Occupation is pre- sumed to reflect the skills that individuals possess as they perform the many maintenance functions of society. Education is believed to 64 reflect both knowledge and cultural taste. The occupational scale is divided into seven groups. Higher executives, proprietors of large concerns and major professionals are given a score of 1, proprietors of medium- si zed businesses and lesser professionals are assigned a score of 2, etc., and unskilled employees are assigned a score of 7. Each category contains a detailed list of occupational titles so that assignment of an occupational score can be determined objectively. The educational scale is also divided into seven groups. Persons who have completed a recognized professional course leading to a graduate degree are given a score of 1, those who have completed a standard college or university program leading to a college degree are given a score of 2, etc., and those who have completed less than seven years of schooling are given a score of 7. The factors of occupation and education are combined to form an Index of Social Position score by multiplying each scale score by a designated factor weight and summing the two resulting scores. Procedure During the data collection period which began December 1, 1981, and ended June 15, 1982, all individuals who requested outpatient counseling at the Palm Beach County Community Mental Health Center were screened during the initial intake interview to determine if they met the criteria for inclusion in the research. To be eligible a client must be judged by the interviewer to be nonpsychotic and must not have been in counseling during the preceding six months. Determination of the client's mental status was made from be- havioral observation based on the Mental Status Schedule (Spitzer, Fleiss, Endicott & Cohen, 1967). Specifically the client must: 1) be 65 oriented as to time, place and person; 2) not display bizarre appear- ance or behavior (engage in rituals, talk to self, posture, grimace); 3) not show overt signs of agitation (i.e. inability to sit, pacing, accelerated speech); 4) not show evidence of disorganized thought processes (i.e. aimless or excessively detailed speech, juxtaposition of ideas with no logical relationship, rapid changes of topic so that ideas are incomplete, irrelevant and/or incoherent); 5) not present evidence of delusions or hallucinations; 6) not be emotionally with- drawn to the extent of showing visible signs of retarded speech and movement, lack of awareness of surroundings, absence of emotional responsivity. The purpose of requiring that a client not have had counseling during the prior six months was to obtain subjects who were actively coping with a stressful situation for which they had not received professional help. At the end of the initial interview, each of the five outpatient therapists, including the researcher, asked new clients who met the inclusion criteria to read the consent form (See Appendix C). Those who agreed to participate by signing the consent form and returning it to the therapist were given a packet which included in the follow- ing order: the demographic information form, the CRI and the ANS-IE. Subjects were instructed to complete the questionnaires in the re- ception room after leaving the therapist's office and to deposit them in a box provided in the reception area. The instructions were repeated on the demographic information form, the first page of the packet. 66 A statement included on the demographic data form reminded sub- jects that the questionnaires did not contain their names and would be seen only by the researcher. This statement and the collection of the completed forms in a box in the reception area were intended to assure the subjects of anonymity, thus increasing the probability of obtaining unbiased responses. Five subjects who were eligible to participate declined to do so. Another seven subjects who agreed to participate did not return the questionnaires. Five more subjects were dropped from the analysis due to failure to respond to all of the questions. One hundred subjects' responses were included in the analysis. Data Analysis Scoring Coping response inventory (CRI). A principal -component factor analysis with varimax rotation was done combining the data from the pilot study factor analysis (n - 143) with the responses of the re- search subjects [n = 100). Seven factors were extracted which ac- counted for 59% of the total variance. The results are summarized in Table 3. Of the seven factors extracted, five defined the response cate- gories of the study. Factor 1 represented direct action responses, Factor 2 represented functional somatic-oriented responses, Factor 3 represented avoidance responses and Factor 4 represented defensive responses of the type described by Lazarus. Factors 5 and 6 repre- sented two forms of potentially dysfunctional somatic-oriented re- sponses. Factor 5 represented the use of medications as a response 67 Table 3 Varimax Rotated Factor Matrix Factor Item Item Loading Direct action factor Talk the problem over with a friend or family member. .51 Try analyzing the problem. .73 Find out everything I can about the situation. .79 Consider all the alternatives. -75 Try to make a decision. .74 Functional somatic-oriented factor Read or listen to music. .35 Exercise--take a walk, jog, swim, dance. .64 Garden or work in the yard. .73 Read about how others have solved similar problems. .34a Do some work with my hands (sew, paint, do carpentry, etc.). -65 Avoidance factor 3 Take a hot bath or shower. -62 Get away from the problem by watching T.V. Eat more. Defensive factor 61 .58 Daydream or think about more pleasant things. .70 Focus on the concerns of others instead of my problem. .63 Decide that the problem doesn't really affect me. .73 Believe that things will take care of themselves. .63b Ignore the problem. -42 Read about how others have solved similar problems. .36 Table 3 - continued Factor Item Item Loading Potentially dysfunctional somatic-oriented type A factor 5 Take tranquil izing medicines. .84 Take sleeping medicines. .85 Potentially dysfunctional somatic-oriented type B factor 6 Believe that things will take care of themselves. .41 Drink more alcohol. .65 Smoke more. .68 Other somatic-oriented responses 7 Try to relax or rest. .70 Read or listen to music. .59 Sleep a lot. .41 Exercise--walk, jog, swim, dance. .34 Note: Includes factors with eigenvalues greater than 1. dItem loaded approximately equally on two factors and was dropped. bItem correlated signigicantly with two factors and was assigned to defensive factor. 69 to stress and Factor 6 represented drinking alcohol and smoking. Factor 7 represented somatic-oriented responses which did not fit the researcher's definitions of either functional or dysfunctional. Be- cause Factors 3 and 7 did not fit into the categories defined by Lazarus, they were not used in testing the hypotheses of the study. Factors 1, 2, 4, 5 and 6 were used in forming the subscales. Items which loaded .30 or greater on each of these factors were com- bined to form five subscales. The item "Read about how others solved similar problems" was dropped because it loaded approximately equally on both Factors 2 and 4. "Believe that things will take care of them- selves," which also loaded significantly on two factors (.63 on Factor 4, .41 on Factor 6), was retained as a Factor 4 item. Table 4 summa- rizes the five subscales used in the hypothesis testing. Subscale scores were derived by summing item scores for each subject. Item scores ranged from 0-6 with 0 points scored for a response of "Never" and 6 points for a response of "Always". Sub- scales had the following range of scores: 1) 0-30; 2) 0-24; 3) 0-24; 4) 0-12; 5) 0-12. Adult Nowicki-Strickland internal-external control scale (ANS-IE). The ANS-IE is scored in the direction of externality such that higher scores represent greater external control orientation. Scores were not dichotomized into groups of externals and internals, rather the continuum of scores was used to prevent the loss of data. Listed below are the 40 items which make up the ANS-IE. One point was scored for a "Yes" response on the items which are preceded by a (Y) and one point was scored for a "No" response on the items which are preceded by a (N). Zero points were scored for other responses. 70 Table 4 Subscales Used in Hypothesis Testing lumber Item Direct action responses Talk the problem over with a friend or family member. Try analyzing the problem. Find out everything I can about the situation. Consider all the alternatives. Try to make a decision. Defensive responses Focus on the concerns of others instead of my problem. Decide that the problem doesn't really affect me. Believe that things will take care of themselves. Ignore the problem. Functional somatic-oriented responses Read or listen to music. Exercise--take a walk, jog, swim, dance. Garden or work in the yard. Do some work with my hands (sew, paint, do carpentry, etc.) 4 Potentially dysfunctional somatic-oriented type A responses Take tranquil i zing medicines, Take sleeping medicines. 5 Potentially dysfunctional somatic-oriented type B responses Drink more alcohol Smoke more. 71 (Y) Do you believe that most problems will solve themselves if you just don't fool with them? (N) Do you believe that you can stop yourself from catching a cold? (Y) Are some people just born lucky? (N) Most of the time did you feel that getting good grades meant a great deal to you? (Y) Are you often blamed for things that just aren't your fault? (N) Do you believe that if somebody studies hard enough he or she can pass any subject? (Y) Do you feel that most of the time it doesn't pay to try hard because things never turn out right anyway? (Y) Do you feel that if things start out well in the morning that it's going to be a good day no matter what you do? (N) Do you feel that most of the time parents listen to what their children have to say? (Y) Do you believe that wishing can make good things happen? (Y) When you get punished does it usually seem it's for no good reason at all ? (Y) Most of the time do you find it hard to change a friend's (mind) opinion? (N) Do you think that cheering more than luck helps a team to win? (Y) Did you feel that it was nearly impossible to change your parent's mind about anything? (N) Do you believe that parents should allow children to make most of their own decisions? (Y) Do you feel that when you do something wrong there's very little you can do to make it right? 72 (Y) Do you believe that most people are just born good at sports? (Y) Are most of the other people your age stronger than you are? (Y) Do you feel that one of the best ways to handle most problems is just not to think about them? (N) Do you feel that you have a lot of choice in deciding whom your friends are? (Y) If you find a four leaf clover, do you believe that it might bring you good luck? (N) Did you often feel that whether or not you did your homework had much to do with what kind of grades you got? (Y) Do you feel that when a person is angry at you, there's little you can do to stop him or her? (Y) Have you ever had a good luck charm? (N) Do you believe that whether or not people like you depends on how you act? (N) Did your parents usually help you if you asked them to? (Y) Have you felt that when people were angry with you it was usually for no reason at all? (N) Most of the time, do you feel that you can change what might happen tomorrow by what you do today? (Y) Do you believe that when bad things are going to happen they just are going to happen no matter what you try do do to stop them? (N) Do you think that people can get their own way if they just keep trying? (Y) Most of the time do you find it useless to try to get your own way at home? 73 (N) Do you feel that when good things happen they happen because of hard work? (Y) Do you feel that when somebody wants to be your enemy there's little you can do to change matters? (N) Do you feel that it's easy to get friends to do what you want them to do? (Y) Do you usually feel that you have little to say about what you get to eat at home? (Y) Do you feel that when someone doesn't like you there's little you can do about it? (Y) Did you usually feel that it was almost useless to try in school because most other children were just plain smarter than you were? (N) Are you the kind of person who believes that planning ahead makes things turn out better? (Y) Most of the time, do you feel that you have little to say about what your family decides to do? (N) Do you think it's better to be smart than to be lucky? Socioeconomic status (SES). Information from the demographic information form relating to years of education and occupation was used to compute an Index of Social Position according to the Hoi lings- head Two Factor Index of Social Position. Each subject was assigned an occupational scale score between 1 and 7 based on the listing of occu- pational titles provided by the Hollingshead Index and an educational scale score between 1 and 7 also based on criteria specified by the Hollingshead Index. In accordance with Holl ingshead 's formula the two factors, occupation and education, were combined to form the Index of Social Position by multiplying each scale score by the following 74 factor weights: occupation, 7; education, 4, and summing the resulting weighted scores. The range of scores is from 11 to 77 with lower scores representing higher SES. The Index of Social Position scores can be arranged on a continuum or divided into social class groups. For the data analysis the continuum of scores was used. Statistical Analysis A partial correlation analysis was done using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) subprogram Partial Corr (Nie, Hull, Jenkins, Steinbrenner & Bent, 1970). Partial correlation pro- vides a single measure of association describing the relationship be- tween two variables, while statistically adjusting for the effects of one or more control variables. The linear effect of the control vari- ables is removed from both independent and dependent variables, cre- ating adjusted independent and dependent variables which are uncor- rected with all control variables which have been entered. Locus of control was defined as the independent variable. Five dependent variables were defined--direct action, defensive, functional somatic-oriented, potentially dysfunctional somatic-oriented Type A (medication) and Type B (drinking alcohol and smoking) responses. Sex and SES, two variables shown to relate consistently to coping behavior, were defined as control variables. Three partial -correlation coef- ficients (two first-order and one second-order partial) were computed to determine the relationship between locus of control and each cate- gory of coping behavior after variance contributed by sex and SES had been removed. For example, to determine the relationship between locus of control and direct action responses the following partials were computed: 75 First-order partial. Locus of control was correlated with direct action responses while controlling for sex. First-order partial. Locus of control was correlated with direct action responses while controlling for SES. Second-order partial. Locus of control was correlated with direct action responses while controlling for sex and SES simultaneously. Because the hypotheses were directional, one-tailed tests of sta- tistical significance were computed for all partials. CHAPTER IV RESULTS The data indicate that there is a statistically significant but small relationship between locus of control and the strategies for coping with stressful events reported by community mental health outpatient clients. A partial correlation analysis was conducted to assess the relationship between locus of control and coping strat- egies, while controlling for sex and SES. A significant positive linear relationship was found between internal locus of control and choice of direct action and functional somatic-oriented responses. A significant negative linear relationship was found between internal locus of control and defensive responses and one of two types of potentially dysfunctional somatic-oriented responses, the use of medication as a response to stress. No significant relationship was found between internal ity and a second type of potentially dysfunc- tional somatic-oriented response, drinking alcohol and smoking. A significant negative linear relationship was found between external locus of control and choice of direct action and functional somatic-oriented responses. A significant positive relationship was found between externality and choice of defensive responses. Only one of two types of potentially dysfunctional somatic-oriented re- sponses, the use of medications as a response to stressful events, showed a positive significant relationship to externality. Table 5 summarizes the results of the partial correlation analysis. 76 77 Table 5 Correlations Between Locus of Control and Coping Responses Direct action Defensive Functional somatic Dysfunctional somatic A B Simple correlations -.32** .17* -.22** .40** .16 Correlations controlling for sex -.34** .19* -.21* .39** .17* Correlations controlling for SES -.31** .19* -.20* .40** .15 Correlations controlling for sex and SES 33* .20* ,20* .40* 16 *r^ < . 05 **£ < . 01 78 The first row of coefficients represents zero order partial s or the simple relationships between locus of control and each of the coping response patterns. The second and third rows show the first order partial s, which relate locus of control to coping responses while controlling first for sex and then for SES. The last row of coefficients is second order partials, which represent the relation- ships between the independent and dependent variables while control- ling for sex and SES simultaneously. As can be seen, the control variables contributed very little variance to the relationship be- tween the independent variable and the dependent variables. There- fore, the relationship between locus of control and the five coping response styles exists virtually independently of sex or SES. Table 6 presents the relationships between the control variables, sex and SES, between sex and the correlation variables and between SES and the correlational variables. There was a statistically signifi- cant relationship between SES and direct action strategies (r = -.19, df = 98, £ -.03) with subjects of higher social class reporting more of these responses than subjects of lower social classes. Sex was also found to relate significantly to SES (r = .22, df = 98, £ = .02) with males having higher SES scores than females. Direct Action Responses Subjects with low scores on the ANS-IE (high internal ity) chose more direct action responses than those with high scores (high exter- nality) (r = -.33). Internal subjects, more often than their external counterparts, reported talking a problem over with a friend or family member, trying to analyze the problem, finding out everything they could about the situation, considering all the alternatives and trying 79 Table 6 Relationship Between Control Variables and Correlational Variables Sex SES Locus of control .10 .15 Direct action responses .15 -.19* Defensive responses -.13 -.07 Functional somatic-oriented responses -.07 -.14 Dysfunctional somatic-oriented responses Type A .06 .00 Type B -.11 -09 Sex 1-00 .22* SES -22* 1.00 80 to make a decision, as responses to events which caused them to feel tense or upset. Both internals and externals reported using these types of responses more frequently than other coping strategies. Direct action responses accounted for 16.7% of total variance, the highest percentage of variance contributed by a factor of the CRI . Defensive Responses High scores on the ANS-IE (high externality) were significantly related to the choice of behaviors which had the effect of denying the stressor or its personal significance or ameliorating the subject's perception of it (r = .20). Externals, more frequently than internals, reported focusing on the concerns of others instead of their problem (projection), deciding that the problem did not really affect them (rationalization), believing that things would take care of themselves (rationalization) and ignoring the problem (denial). Functional Somatic-Oriented Responses Internals, more than externals, reported using the following re- sponses which have the effect of relieving the dysphoric somatic ac- companiments of stressful events while increasing future capacity for coping (r = -.20): "Read or listen to music," "Excercise--take a walk, jog, swim, dance," "Garden or work in the yard" and "Do some work with my hands (sew, paint, do carpentry, etc.)." Potentially Dysfunctional Somatic-Oriented Responses--Type A High scores on the ANS-IE (externality) were related to use of sleeping pills and tranquilizers (r = .40) as methods of relieving the somatic discomfort associated with anticipating or responding to stressful situations. 81 Potentially Dysfunctional Somatic-Oriented Responses—Type B The relationship between locus of control and increased use of drinking alcohol and smoking to relieve the somatic distress associ- ated with aversive situations failed to reach an acceptable level of significance. However, the relationship was in the expected direction with externals reporting more frequent use of these responses than internals (r = .16, df = 96, £ = .06). CHAPTER V DISCUSSION Locus of control is a statistically significant variable in the choice of coping responses reported by community mental health out- patient clients. As internality increased the use of direct action and functional somatic-oriented responses increased. These findings support Research Hypothesis 1A. As internality increased the use of defensive responses and one type of potentially dysfunctional somatic- oriented response, the use of medication as a response to stress, decreased. There was also a decrease in drinking alcohol and smoking with increased internality; however, this relationship was not statis- tically significant (r = -.16, df = 96, £ = .06). These results only partially support Research Hypothesis IB. As predicted by Hypothesis 2A, greater externality was related to decreased use of direct action and functional somatic-oriented re- sponses. The prediction of Hypothesis 2B, of a significant positive relationship between external locus of control and choice of defensive and potentially dysfunctional somatic-oriented responses, was only partially supported. The hypothesized relationship between external- ity and use of defensive strategies was observed. However, only one of two types of potentially dysfunctional somatic-oriented responses, the use of medication, was significantly and positively related to externality. The relationship between externality and drinking alco- hol and smoking as a response to stress was in the expected direction but failed to reach an acceptable level of significance (r = .16, df = 96, £ = .06). 82 83 The magnitudes of the correlations between locus of control and coping mechanisms, which ranged from .20 for defensive and functional somatic-oriented responses and .40 for potentially dysfunctional somatic-oriented--Type A responses (see Table 5), indicate that although locus of control is a statistically significant variable related to the choice of coping mechanisms it is not a major factor. These findings offer limited support for Lazarus' (1964) theory that personal perception of one's potential for mastery is a sig- nificant factor related to coping strategy. Persons who believed that outcomes were contingent on their behavior (internals) chose more effective coping strategies than those who attributed outcomes to chance, fate or powerful others (externals). Internals used more direct action and fewer defensive responses than externals. Similar results have been reported by Anderson (1977), Brown (1976) and Pearlin and Schooler (1978). These results differ from those of Chamberlain (1980) and Ritter (1980), who found no relationship be- tween locus of control and coping mechanisms. The finding that management of dysphoric somatic accompaniments of stress was also handled more effectively by internals had not been reported previ- ously, as no studies of this type of coping response were found in the 1 i terature. This study supports Lazarus' (1977) theory that coping is a com- plex process which includes some combination of direct action, defen- sive and somatic oriented responses. A number of researchers (Hamburg & Adams, 1967; Mechanic, 1962; Penman, 1980; Sanders & Kardinal, 1977) had observed that persons who coped most effectively with a variety of stressors used a coping style which combined direct action with defensive strategies but relied most on direct action ap- proaches. This pattern was also observed for the subjects of the present study whether they are considered as a group or are looked at in subgroups according to sex, race, marital status, age, years of education, social class, primary presenting problem or referral source (see Table 7). Additionally, the total group and each of the subgroups used more functional somatic-oriented responses than dys- functional somatic-oriented responses. Thus, for these subjects a coping style emerged which included a combination of coping responses used in the following order from highest to lowest frequency: direct action, functional somatic-oriented, defensive and potentially dys- functional somatic-oriented. This pattern remained for the total group and for 28 of the 32 subgroups after combining the means of the two forms of potentially dysfunctional somatic-oriented responses so that the number of items included was equal to the number of items on the defensive and functional somatic-oriented scales. Although there are five items on the direct action subscale, compared with four items on each of the others (after combining the two potentially dysfunc- tional somatic-oriented subscales), one item could not have accounted for the approximate 10-point difference between mean direct action re- sponses and the means of the next most frequently reported responses, functional somatic-oriented responses. Despite repeated past findings that sex and SES were related to direct action and defensive response styles (Brown, 1976; Brown et al . , 1975: Calhoun, 1980; Parent, 1976; Pearlin & Schooler, 1978; Penman, 1980; Tanck & Robbins, 1979; Warheit, 1979), very little of the shared variance between locus of control and coping mechanisms was 85 r— CO A3 o u IT) CO in OO in^j-roo *3- * CM 0O CNJ 0O U o (!) 1. +j i ; o in qd o oo Ln O oo ud r^ O O CO OO LOUJO^t O O CT> CD i — Ln mo oi(\i CD O CO o CO CT. i— O CTi |\ CXl OO cncTicoco co en en cr> i — CM CM i — <3 en en i — CM I I r— OO CO ■3- Ln LONO co r— Ln CD ID <3" oo Ncn, — oo O CD co m WLOCDh- o r-- co oo in co CXl ^j- CM oo cm in en CM CM ^T O cm *ct- o in ^j- Ln O CM u m -a 00 -i- +-> T3 CD T3 CD C CO CD CD O CD CO. — CD-^tO i — -i— S- 5 CD fO -(-> O CLi — cn S- O O i — E -i— rO«/lirJCS->TJ rO CD SZ i — •!— +J ■■— ro ■!— ■!— s u_ cd3cq3:-i-oos:q3 ocnLninM-cMCMiniD oora-inio Oi — i — i — i — i— LD en co en co oo i— en en r-~ CTl CO CTv CO i- +-> •I- (J Q < CD CD CO ^J" o ^a- o o oo i— 00 O CTl OO i — ID i — VO vO LD co r- oo o OO vO vO CD i— oo i— o CvJ ■ — CvJ OvJ r-> o o r» en CvJ C\J i — i — en en o en CD lt) OvJ I— OvJ r— O O UD OO 0O LD OvJ LD I-- vD OvJ O LT) OO vo in ovi co r-« i — OvJ r— r-~ i— , >>-c: oo +-> 4-> +-> 0) -, S- CD ! Z3 O O (0 OJ o. uo co >- S 3 =i E o a; >, t- 4-> o re +j a) cu oo _o CO -i- c: -c s- cu u <_> ■P (0 U) CU O) i— -o cu ai cu +-> >, M- ro i — s- cu Z5 CO o cu o >> CO I — ra o CO cu cu i— +-> ^ D. 1/1 ra o ^ i — 3 CI) r- QJ 0) cl) 4- r0 >- n Cl> u 2: n CO co ec U CI) Q.JZ cnl— +J C •— 1 rn ■r- s: 0) +-> o (I) > CC > QJ S- r— rn O -£_ , — CU -ZL CI) -SZ =5 i- 4-> O o a >! S- C 1 O c -i-> C!) -C T3 E T L0 1- CU ■K ■r- O E C 2 cu 4-> h QJ ra -C -M ~o +-> CO 104 - o 107 APPENDIX B DEMOGRAPHIC DATA FORM DEMOGRAPHIC DATA FORM INSTRUCTIONS The following questionnaires are to be used in a study of the ways that people cope with stressful situations. These forms do not contain your name and will be seen only by the researcher. When you have com- pleted these forms, please drop them in the box marked "Questionnaires" which is located on the table in the reception area of the front lobby. IMPORTANT: Be sure to answer every question--DO NOT OMIT ANY. Years of Education Hispanic Separated or Divorced Widowed Age Male Female Caucasian Negro Marital Status: Never Marri ed Married Occupation: (ANSWER EVEN IF YOU ARE NOT PRESENTLY EMPLOYED; Type of problem for which you requested counseling: Interpersonal relationship (problem with spouse, boyfriend/ girlfriend, family member, roommate, friend, etc.) Depression Anxiety Other (Specify] Who referred you to the Center? Self Family member or friend Another agency (e.g. HRS, Parent Child Study Center, etc. Family physician 109 APPENDIX C INFORMED CONSENT FORM INFORMED CONSENT I am conducting a study to determine the relationship between personality characteristics and the ways that people cope with stress- ful situations. I would like you to fill out three questionnaires which will be given to you by your therapist and deposit them in the box marked "Questionnaires" which is located on the table in the re- ception area of the front lobby. Your name will not appear on the questionnaires nor will they be made a part of your record. It is hoped that the information gained from this study will enable us to better understand the coping process and to provide counseling services which most effectively meet the needs of those who seek professional help in dealing with stressful life events. Your participation in this study is voluntary. No monetary com- pensation will be given. You are free to withdraw your consent and to discontinue participation at any time without prejudice. I will be happy to answer any questions that you have concerning the questionnaires. If you would like to take part in the research described above, please sign this form and return it to your therapist. "I have read and I understand the procedure described above. I agree to participate in the procedure and I have received a copy of this description." Signatures : Subject ~~~" ~ ~ ' """ Date Witness Date Relationship if other Date Margie Stevenson Date than subject 506 Gale Place West Palm Beach, FL 111 REFERENCES Abram, H.S. Adaptation to open heart surgery: a psychiatric study of response to the threat of death. American Journal of Psychiatry, 1965, 122, 659-667. Anderson, C.R. Coping behaviors as intervening mechanisms in the inverted-U stress-performance relationship. Journal of Appl ied Psychology, 1976, 61(1), 30-34. Anderson, C.R. Locus of control, coping behavior, and performance in a stress setting: a longitudinal study. Journal of Applied Psy- chology, 1977, 62(4), 446-451. Andrew, J.N. Recovery from surgery, with or without preparatory in- structions, for three coping styles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1970, 15(3), 223-226. Andrews, G., Tennant, C, Hewson, D.M. , & Vaillant, G.E. Life events, social support, coping style, and risk of psychological impair- ment. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 1978, 166(5) , 307-316. Averill, J.R., & Rosenn, M. Vigilant and nonvigilant coping strategies and psychophysiological stress reactions during the anticipation of electric shock. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1972, 23(1), 128-141. Baker, E.K. The relationship between locus of control and psycho- therapy. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, 1979, 16(3), 351-362. Barber, C.A. Coping with the stress of life change: how some people stay healthy. Dissertation Abstracts International, 1979, 40(6), 2890B. Boyd, I., Yeager, M. , & McMillan. Personality styles in the post- operative course. Psychosomatic Medicine, 1973, 35(1 ) , 23-40. Brannigan, G.C., Rosenberg, L.A., & Loprete, L.J. Internal -external expectancy, maladjustment and psychotherapeutic intervention. Journal of Personality Assessment, 1977, 41_, 71-78. Brown, P. Psychological distress and personal growth among women coping with marital dissolution. (Doctoral dissertation, Uni- versity of Michigan, 1976). 112 113 Brown, G.W., Bhrolchain, M. , & Harris, T. Social class and psychiatric disturbance among women in an urban population. Sociology, 1975, 9, 225-254. Byrne, D. The repression-sensi tization scale: rationale, reliability, and validity. Journal of Personality, 1961, 29_, 334-349. Calhoun, R.W. Stress, strain, and coping styles among married graduate students and their spouses. Dissertation Abstracts International, 1980, 40(7), 3361 B. Carter, S.R. The relationship of repression-sensitization to cognitive and behavioral indices in a success-failure paradigm. Disser- tation Abstracts International, 1974, 34(11), 5671 B. Chamberlain, D.C. Sources of stress, psychological structure and coping response. Dissertation Abstracts International, 1980, 40(7), 3459B. Chandler, T.A. A note on the relationship of internal ity-external ity, self-acceptance, and self-ideal discrepancies. Journal of Psy- chology, 1976, _94, 145-146. Chodoff, P., Friedman, S.B., & Hamburg, D.A. Stress, defenses and coping behavior: observations in parents of children with malignant disease. American Journal of Psychiatry, 1964, 120(8), 743-749. Coelho, G.V., Silber, E. , & Hamburg, D.A. Use of the Student-TAT to assess coping behavior in hospitalized, normal, and exceptually competent college freshmen. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1962, T_4, 355-365. Cohen, F. , & Lazarus, R.S. Active coping processes, coping dispositions, and recovery from surgery. Psychosomatic Medicine, 1973, 35(5) , 375-389. Coleman, J.C. Life stress and maladaptive behavior. The American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 1973, 27(4), 169-180. Connolly, S.G. Changing expectancies: a counseling model based on locus of control. Personnel and Guidance Journal , 1980, 59(3) , 176-180. Cowie, B. The cardiac patient's perception of his heart attack. Social Science and Medicine, 1976, JO, 87-96. Deaton, J.E., Berg, S.W. , Richlin, M. , & Litrownik, A.J. Coping activities in solitary confinement of US Navy P0WS in Vietnam. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 1977, 7(3), 239-257. 114 DeLong, R.D. Individual differences in patterns of anxiety arousal, stress-relevant information and recovery from surgery. Disserta- tion Abstracts International, 1971, 32(1), 554-555B. Dinardo, Q.E. Psychological adjustment to spinal cord injury. Disser- tation Abstracts International, 1972, 32(7), 4206-4207B. Diskin, S.D., Goldstein, M.J., & Grencik, J.M. Coping patterns of law enforcement officers in simulated and naturalistic stress. American Journal of Community Psychology, 1977, 5(1 ) , 59-73. Dohrenwend, B.S., & Dohrenwend, B.P. Class and race as status-related sources of stress. The study of stress. Chicago: Aldine Press, 1967. Dohrenwend, B.S., & Dohrenwend, B.P. Some issues in research on stress- ful life events. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 1978, 166(1), 7-15. " Duke, M.P., & Mullens, C. Interpersonal distance as a function of locus of control in hospitalized schizophrenics and non-schizophre- nics. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1973, 41 , 230-234. Fenz, W.D. Strategies for coping with stress. In I.G. Sarason & CD. Spielberger, (Eds. ) , Stress and anxiety (Vol. 2). New York: John Wiley & Sons, 1975, 305-336. Ford, C.V. The Pueblo incident: psychological response to severe stress. In I.G. Sarason & CD. Spielberger, (Eds.), Stress and anxiety (Vol. 2). New York: John Wiley & Sons, 1975, 229-241. Friedrich, W.N. Predictors of the coping behavior of mothers of handi- capped children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1979, 47(6), 1140-1141. Gal, R. The effects of various coping activities on reactions during a stressful anticipatory period. Dissertation Abstracts Internation- al_, 1976, 37(1), 437-438B. Goldstein, M.J. The relationship between coping and avoiding behavior and response to fear arousing propaganda. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1959, 58, 247-252. "~ " Haan, N. Coping and defense mechanisms related to personality inven- tories. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 1964, 29(4), 373-378. Hamburg, D.A. , & Adams, J.E. A perspective on coping behavior. Ar- chives of General Psychiatry, 1967, 1_7, 277-284. Hamburg, D.A., Hamburg, B. , & deGoza, S. Adaptive problems and mecha- nisms in severely burned patients. Psychiatry, 1953, 16(1 ) , 1-20. 115 Hollingshead, A.B. Two-factor index of social position. New Haven, Conn., 1957. Holmes, T.H., & Rahe, R.H. The social readjustment rating scale. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 1967, _T1_, 213-218. Horn, M.L. The integration of negative experience by high and low functioning women. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Florida, 1975). Houston, B.K., & Hodges, W.F. Situational denial and performance under stress. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1970, 16(4), 726-730. Jam's, I. Psychological stress. New York: John Wiley & Sons, 1958. Janis, I. Psychodynamic aspects of stress tolerance. In S.Z. Klausner (Ed.), The quest for self control. New York: The Free Press, 1965, 215-246. Janis, I. When fear is healthy. Psychology Today, 1968, 1, pp. 46-49; 60-61. Joe, V.C. Review of the internal -external control construct as a personality variable. Psychological Reports, 1971, 28, 619-640. Katz, J.L., Weiner, H., Gallagher, T.F., & Hellman, L. Stress, dis- tress, and ego defenses. Archives of General Psychiatry, 1970, 23, 131-142. Lazarus, R.S. A laboratory approach to the dynamics of psychological stress. American Psychologist, 1964, 19(6), 400-411. Lazarus, R.S. Psychological stress and coping in adaptation and illness. International Journal of Psychiatry in Medicine, 1974, 5(4), 321-333. ~ ~ * " Lazarus, R.S. Patterns of adjustment and human effectiveness. New York: McGraw-Hill Book Co. , 1977. '" Lazarus, R.S. Personal communication, December 8, 1980. Lazarus, R.S. The costs and benefits of denial. In S. Breznitz (Ed.), Denial of stress. New York: International Universities Press, in press. Lazarus, R.S., Averill, J.R., & Opton, J.E. The psychology of coping: issues of research and assessment. In G.V. Coelho, D.A. Hamburg, & J.E. Adams (Eds.), Coping and adaptation. New York: Basic Books, 1974, 249-315. ~ ~ 116 Lazarus, R.S., & Launier, R. Stress-related transactions between per- son and environment. In L.A. Pervin & M. Lewis (Eds.), Perspec- tives in interactional psychology. New York: Plenum Publishing Corp., 1978, 287-327. Lefcourt, H.M. Internal versus external control of reinforcement: a review. Psychological Bulletin, 1966, 65(4), 206-220. Lefcourt, H.M. The functions of the illusions of control and freedom. American Psychologist, 1973, 28, 417-425. Lefcourt, H.M. Locus of control and the response to aversive events. Canadian Psychological Review, 1976, 17(3), 202-209. Lefcourt, H.M. , Gronnerud, P., & McDonald, P. Cognitive activity and hypothesis formation during a double entendre word association test as a function of locus of control and field dependence. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 1973, 5, 161-173. MacCornack, V.A. Rate of recovery from life-threatening surgery as a function of coping style. Dissertation Abstracts International, 1979, 40(6), 2877-2878B. Mechanic, D. Students under stress: a study in the social psychology of adaptation. New York: Free Press of Glencoe, 1962. Menninger, K. Regulatory devices of the ego under major stress. Inter- national Journal of Psychoanalysis, 1954, ^35, 412-420. Nie, N.H., Hull, C.H., Jenkins, J.G., Steinbrenner, K. , & Bent, D.H. Statistical package for the social sciences: second edition. New York: McGraw-Hill Book Company, 1970, 301-319. Niemela, P. Coping patterns in shock anticipation and in everyday stress. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 1974, J_5, 268-272. Nowicki, S. A manual for the adult Nowicki-Strickland locus of control scale. Emory University, 1979. Nowicki, S. , & Duke, M.P. A locus of control scale for college as well as noncollege adults. Journal of Personality Assessment, 1974, 38, 136-137. Nowicki, S., & Strickland, B.R. A locus of control scale for children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1973, 40, 148-154. Overbeck, A.L. Life stress antecedents to application for help at a mental health center: a clinical study of adaptation. Smith College Studies in Social Work, 1977, 47(3), 192-233. Parent M K Stress, coping and growth in middle age. Dissertation Abstracts International, 1976, 37(2), 1229-1230A. 117 Pearlin, L.I., & Schooler, C. The structure of coping. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 1978, _19_, 2-21. Penman, D.T. Coping strategies in adaptation to mastectomy. Disserta- tion Abstracts International, 1980, 40(12) , 5825B . Perlman, H.H. In quest of coping. Social Casework, 1975, 56(4) , 213- 225. Rabkin, J.G., & Struening, E.L. Life events, stress and illness. Science, 1976, 194, 1013-1020. Rippere, V. Anti depressive behavior: a preliminary report. Behavior Research and Therapy, 1976, IA, 289-299. Ritter, R.L. Preoperative stress, coping style, and locus of control in cardiac surgery patients. Dissertation Abstracts International, 1980, 40(7) , 3373-3374B. " " Robbins, P.R. Personal communication, December 6, 1980. Rosenberg, H.S. Differences in coping skills, life events and social support between relapsed and non-relapsed alcohol abusers. Dis- sertation Abstracts International, 1980, 40(10) , 5018B. Rotter, J.B. Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. Psychological Monographs, 1966, 80(1, Whole No. 609). " ""*" Sanders, J.B., & Kardinal , C.G. Adaptive coping mechanisms in adult acute leukemia patients in remission. Journal of American Medical Association, 1977, 238(9) , 952-954. Sax, G. Principles of educational measurement and evaluation. Cali- fornia: Wadsworth Publishing Co., Inc., 1974. Selye, H. The stress of life. New York: McGraw-Hill Book Co., Inc., 1956. Selye, H. The stress of life: revised edition. New York: McGraw- Hill Book Co., Inc. , 1976. Spitzer, R.L., Fleiss, J.L., Endicott, J.E. & Cohen, J. Mental status schedule. Archives of General Psychiatry, 1967, _16, 479-493. Stensrud, R. , & Stensrud, K. Counseling may be hazardous to your health: how we teach people to feel powerless. Personnel and Guidance Journal , 1981, 59(5) , 300-304. Tanck, R.H., & Robbins, P.R. Asserti veness , locus of control and coping behaviors used to diminish tension. Journal of Personality Assessment, 1979, 43(4) , 396-400. 118 Teichman, Y. The stress of coping with the unknown regarding a signi- ficant family member. In I.G. Sarason and CD. Spielberger (Eds.) Stress and anxiety (Vol. 2). New York: John Wiley & Sons, 1975, 243-255. Thelen, M.H. & Varble, D.L. Comparison of college students seeking psychotherapy with nontherapy students on coping and defense scales. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 1970, 26(1), 123-124. Visotsky, H.M. , Hamburg, D.A. , Goss, M.E., & Lebovits, B.Z. Coping behavior under extreme stress. Archives of General Psychiatry, 1961,5,423-448. " " '" " "~ " '" Warheit, G.I. Life events, coping, stress and depressive symptomato- logy. American Journal of Psychiatry, 1979, 136(4B), 502-507. Wolk, S., & Kurtz, J. Positive adjustment and involvement during aging and expectancy for internal control. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 1975, 43(2), 173-178. "" BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH Margie K. Stevenson was born on August 16, 1939, in New Orleans Louisiana. She received the Bachelor of Arts degree from the Univer- sity of Florida with a major in psychology in August, 1975. She and her husband have two children, Michael and Pamela. 119 I certify that I have read this study and that in my opinion it conforms to acceptable standards of scholarly presentation and is fully adequate, in scope and quality, as a dissertation for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy. Ju. ./-.— jL a- Theodore Landsman, Chairman Professor of Counselor Education I certify that I have read this study and that in my opinion it conforms to acceptable standards of scholarly presentation and is fully adequate, in scope and quality, as a dissertation for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy. M Uc. Ruth H. Alexander Professor of Physical Education I certify that I have read this study and that in my opinion it conforms to acceptable standards of scholarly presentation and is fully adequate, in scope and quality, as a dissertation for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy. ,x/~v ,v-cx QJU: kp^qs^ Janies Algina Associate Professor o of Education Foundations I certify that I have read this study and that in my opinion it conforms to acceptable standards of scholarly presentation and is fully adequate, in scope and quality, as a dissertation for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy. / David Suchman Associate Professor of Psychology This dissertation was submitted to the Graduate Faculty of the Depart- ment of Counselor Education in the College of Education and to the Graduate Council, and was accepted as partial fulfillment of the re- quirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy. December, 1982 Dean for Graduate Studies and Research ^'VERS'Ty OF FLORIDA 3 1262 08553 1209