BLM LIBRARY ilium ■ii 8802675" SNAKE RIVER BIRDS OF PREY RESEARCH PROJECT ', . ANNUAL REPORT 1976 U.S. DEPARTMENT OF THE INTERIOR BUREAU OF LAND MANAGEMENT JOISE DISTRICT IDAHO ~:- Bureau Of Land Management Denver Service Center As the Nation's principal conservation agency, the Department of the Interior has responsibility for most of our nationally owned public lands and natural resources. This includes fostering the wisest use of our land and water resources, protecting our fish and wildlife, preserving the environmental and cultural values of our national parks and historical places, and providing for the enjoyment of life through outdoor recreation. The Department assesses our energy and mineral resources and works to assure that their development is in the best interests of all our people. The Department also has a major responsibility for American Indian reservation communities and for people who live in Island Territories under U.S. administration. ^£79? :0:00 «V. M. Qi ^. s 30 0 o E '1", 0 a. 20 10 I, Jaclcrabbit Cottontail J3 Agricultui = Intermediate Range ^ Townsend Pheasant Ground Squirrel I I Othe U) ^ig. 5. Food habits of golden eagles in relation to land use, 197f 34 Table 15. Prairie falcon food habits in the Snake River Birds of Prey Study Area, 1976.* Species Class Number of Individuals Estimated Total Biomass (g) Percent of Total Prey Percent of Total Biomass MAMMALS Townsend ground squirrel A (Male) 2 520 0.5 0.9 A 2 422 0.5 0.7 J 1 106 0.2 0.2 II 258 41 ,538 61.8 72.1 t .otal 263 42 ,586 62.9 73.9 Deer Mouse U 51 867 12.2 1.5 Meadow Mouse U 18 1 ,080 4.3 1.9 Kangaroo Rat, Unid U 8 444 1.9 0.8 Townsend Pocket CTopher U 7 1 ,400 1.7 2.4 Cottontail u 6 3 ,084 1.4 9.4 Black-tailed Jackrabbit D 4 5 ,516 1.0 5.4 Harvest Mouse U 5 75 1.2 0.1 Antelope Ground Squirrel U 3 318 0.7 0.6 Pocket Gopher Unid u 2 410 0.5 0.7 Woodrat, Unid. u 2 406 0.5 0.7 Desert Woodrat u 1 118 0.2 0.2 Shrew, Unid. u 1 18 0.2 + Mouse, Unid. 0 1 20 0.2 + SUBTOTAL 372 56 ,342 89.0 97.8 BIRDS Western Meadowlark u 4 384 1.0 0.7 Sparrow, Unid. u 4 68 1.0 0.1 California Quail u 1 198 0.2 0.3 Mourning Dove u 1 113 0.2 0.2 Brewer's Blackbird u 1 68 0.2 0.1 Red-winged Blackbird u 1 54 0.2 0.1 Sage Sparrow u 1 30 0.2 + White-crowned Sparrow u 1 30 0.2 + Horned Lark u 1 28 0.2 + Yellow Warbler u 1 20 0.2 + SUBTOTAL 16 993 3.8 1.7 REPTILES Whiptail Lizard u 9 135 2.2 0.2 Horned Lizard u 2 36 0.5 0.1 Leopard Lizard u 1 23 0.2 + Lizard, Unid. SUBTOTAL u 1 13 17 211 0.2 3.1 + 0T4 INVERTEBRATES Beetle u 10 30 2.4 + Scorpion SUBTOTAL u 7 17 10 40 1.7 4.1 + 0.1 TOTAL PREY ITEMS 418 57,586 * See appendix for assigned weights and scientific names + = Trace A = Adult M = Medium Juvenile Unknown 35 Table 16. Red-tailed hawk food habits in the Snake River Birds of Prey- Study Area, 1976.* Species Size Class Number of Individuals MAMMALS Townsend Ground Squirrel A(Male ) 6 A(Fema le)4 A 7 M 10 J 5 U total 157 189 Townsend Pocket Gopher A 3 U 41 total 44 Cottontail A 2 M 3 J 6 U 23 total 34 Black-tailed jackrabbit U 11 Kangaroo Rat, Uni d u 7 Harvest Mouse u 5 Yellow-bellied Marmot J 1 u 3 total 4 Woodrat, Unid. U 3 Deer Mouse u 3 Muskrat M J U 1 total 2 Domestic. Cat U 1 Pocket Gopher, Una d u 1 Long-tailed Wease 1 u 1 Meadow Mouse u 1 Estimated Total Biomass(g) SUBTOTAL 306 1,560 748 1,477 1,670 530 25,277 31,262 720 8,200 8,920 1,654 1,500 1,290 11,822 16,266 15,169 388 75 50n 5,001 5,501 609 51 1,052 1,157 2,209 1,800 205 178 60 82,693 Percent of Total Prey 1.4 1.0 1.7 2.4 1 .2 37.9 45.7 0.7 9.9 10.6 0.5 0.7 8.2 2.7 1 .7 1.2 0.2 0.7 1.0 0.7 0.7 0.2 0.2 0.2 0.? Percent of Total Biomass 1.6 0.8 1.5 1 .7 0.6 26.3 32.5 0.7 8.5 9.3 1.7 1. 1. 12. 16.9 15.9 0.4 0.1 73.9 1.1 1.2 2.3 1.9 0.2 0.2 0.1 36.0 36 Table 16. Continued Number Estimated Percent Percent Size of Total of Total of Total Species Class Individual s Biomass (g) Prey Biomass BIRDS Passerine, Unid. U 8 136 1.9 0.1 Rock D ove U 3 900 0.7 0.9 Chukar A 2 1,142 0.5 1.2 Cliff Swallow A 2 30 0.5 + Ring-necked Pheasant U 1 1,199 0.2 1.2 Mallard A 1 1,148 0.2 1.2 Gray Partridge A 1 376 0.2 0.4 Califorr i.a Quail A 1 198 0.2 0.2 Black-billed Magpie M 1 173 0.2 0.2 Mourning Dove M 1 113 0.2 0.1 SUBTOTAL 21 5,415 5.1 5.6 REPTILES Gopher Snake A U total Ground Snake U Striped Whipsnake U Whiptail Lizard U Lizard, Unid. U Rattlesnake U Snake, Unid. U Reptile, Unid. U Racer V Western Fence Lizard U Night Snake u SUBTOTAL INVERTEBRATES Beetle u Scorpion u Grasshopper u Spider u SUBTOTAL TOTAL PREY ITEMS 7 33 40 6 5 4 2 1 1 1 1 1 1 63 18 3 2 1 24 414 1.911 4,818 6,729 48 485 60 34 329 184 101 68 17 14_ 8,069 5 4 1 1 11 96,189 1.7 8.0 9.7 1.4 1.2 1.0 0.5 0.2 0.2 0.2 0.2 0.2 0.2 15.2 4.3 0.7 0.5 0.2 2.0 5.0 7.0 + 0.5 0.1 + 0.3 0.2 0.1 0.1 + + 8.4 + + + + *See appendix for assigned weights and scientific names, A = Adult J = Juvenile + = Trace M = Medium U = Unknown (average) 37 (Thomomys spp.), not previously identified in food habit collections, comprised about 10 percent by numbers and biomass in 1976. Pocket gopher remains were identified in five of seven sites, and lagomorph remains occurred at all sites studied. Hawks, nesting near farmland, captured more pocket gophers than hawks nesting near range lands. Ferruginous hawks. Townsend pocket gophers (Thomomys townsendii) and Townsend ground squirrels comprised 54 percent of prey numbers and 90 percent of prey biomass at the four ferruginous hawk nests studied (Table 17) . Adult ferruginous hawks remove castings and prey remains from their nests (L. Powers pers. comm. ) . This decreases the accuracy of the four day interval collections. Ferruginous hawk food habits were last studied in 1973 when a small number of collections at two nests identified mostly lagomorphs, woodrats (Neotoma spp.), and Townsend ground squirrels. In 1976, more pocket gophers were found in nests located near farmlands than those in range land areas. Ravens . As in previous years, mammals comprised almost all the prey biomass identified in castings collected at raven nests in 1976 (Table 18). Townsend ground squirrels were still most prevalent. However, their biomass contribution decreased from 93 to 70 percent between 1975 and 1976 due to a decrease in the frequency of occurrence of ground squirrels in the diet, an increase in prey diversity and an increase in a small number of lagomorphs that contribute a large biomass. This may have been an artifact of our sampling procedure. Young ravens at the Murphy rim nest consumed large numbers of beetles and scorpions. Some ground squirrel remains were found here late in the fledgling period although there are no known ground squirrel demes nearby. Townsend ground squirrels were the most commonly found prey species at all other nests regardless of habitat or land use practice. Raptor Census We ran monthly road transects in 1976 as in 1975, 1974, and 1973. Monthly totals varied widely for some species between years. As in past years, ravens were the most numerous. The number of rough-legged .hawks observed from January through March was one fourth of the number counted during the same period in 1975. We also counted fewer red-tailed hawks than in 1975. Golden eagle, prairie falcon and kestrel numbers remained relatively constant (Table 19) . Average yearly results (Table 20) for all species by month show general trends of density. Golden eagles, prairie falcons, red-tailed hawks, kestrels, marsh hawks, ravens and shrikes (Lanius spp.) occupy the BPSA year round. Rough-legged hawks (Buteo lagopus) are fall and winter residents. Merlins (Falco columbarius) , Cooper's hawks (Accipter cooperii) , and goshawks (Accipter gentilis) winter or migrate through in very low numbers 3b Table 17. Ferruginous hawk food habits in the Snake River Birds of Prey Study Area, 1976.* Species Number Size of Class Individuals Estimated Percent Percent of Total of Total Biomass (g) Total Prey Biomass MAMMALS Townsend Ground Squirrel A 2 422 1.7 M 1 167 0.9 U 32 5,152 27.4 total 35 5,741 29.9 Townsend Pocket ■ Gopher A 16 3,840 13.7 LI 12 2,400 10.3 total 28 6,240 23.9 Kangaroo Rat,Unid. U 2 11.1 1.7 Mouse, Unid. U 2 40 1.7 Cottontail U Woodrat, Unid. U SUBTOTAL BIRDS Rock Wren U Say's Phoebe U Passerine, Unid. U SUBTOTAL REPTILES Western Fence Lizard I! Whiptail Lizard U Woodhouse's Toad U Lizard, Unid. U Snake, Unid. U Reptile, Unid. U SUBTOTAL INVERTEBRATES 69 1 1 1 5 2 _1 11 Beetle U 28 Grasshopper U 2 Jerusalem Cricket U 1 Cicada u 1 Wasp u 1 SUBTOTAL 33 TOTAL PREY ITEMS 117 514 40 12,849 13,279 0.9 0.9 59.0 15 1.7 21 0.9 17 53 0.9 3.4 17 0.9 15 0.9 20 0.9 17 4.3 184 1.7 101 0.9 354 9.4 18 23.9 1 1.7 2 0.9 1 0.9 1 0.9 23 28.2 3.2 1.3 38.8 43.2 28.9 18.1 47.0 0.8 0.3 3.9 0.3 96.8 0.1 0.2 0.1 0.4 0.1 0.1 0.2 0.1 1.4 0.8 2.7 0.1 + + + 0.1 " See appendix for assigned weights and scientific names. A = Adult J = Juvenile + = Trace M = Medium U = Unknown (Average) 39 Table 18. Raven food habits in the Snake River Birds of Prey Study Area, 1976. ; Species*- Number of Individual s Estimated Total Biomass (g) MAMMALS Townsend Ground Squirrel Deer Mouse Kangaroo Rat, Unid. Townsend Pocket Gopher Cottontail Black-tailed Jackrabbit Desert Woodrat Woodrat, Unid. Harvest Mouse Great Basin Kangaroo Rat Mouse, Unid. House Mouse SUBTOTAL BIRDS Passerine Egg Sparrow, Unid. SUBTOTAL REPTILES and AMPHIBIANS Side-blotched Lizard Gopher Snake Striped Wbipsnake Collared Lizard Toad, Unid. Horned Lizard Whiptail Lizard SUBTOTAL INVERTEBRATES Beetle Scorpion Grasshopper SUBTOTAL 163 12 10 7 6 3 3 2 2 1 1 1_ 211 26,234 204 555 1,400 084 137 354 406 30 65 20 15 36,513 Percent of Total Prey 21.1 1.7 1.4 1.0 0.8 0.4 0.4 0.3 0.3 0.1 0.1 0.1 29.3 Percent of Total Biomass 70.3 0.5 1.5 3.8 8.3 11.1 0.1 + 97.8 2 4 0.3 l 1 17 0.1 + 3 21 0.4 0.1 3 12 0.4 1 2 292 0.3 0.8 1 97 0.1 0.3 1 26 0.1 0.1 1 20 0.1 0.1 1 18 0.1 + 1 11 15 497 0.1 1.5 + 173 360 108 50.1 0.3 132 191 18.4 0.5 2 2 0.3 + 494 301 68.7 0.8 TOTAL PREY ITEMS 719 37,332 * See appendix for assigned weights and scientific names. "'"'All prey items unknown (average) size class. + = trace 40 Table 19 . Birds of prey observed on the monthly raptor road transect in the Birds of Prey Natural Area, 1976. Number of Bi rds Species Jan Feb Mar Apr May Jun Jul Aug Sep Oct Nov Dec Golden Eagle 4 3 3 1 9 3 2 3 3 Prairie Falcon 3 1 5 2 2 4 1 1 2 7 4 Kestrel 4 8 2 2 1 3 4 5 13 6 6 8 Red-tailed Hawk 1 1 1 1 1 3 3 2 1 Rough-legged Hawk 4 1 3 2 5 7 Swainson's Hawk 2 1 Cooper's Hawk 1 1 1 Marsh Hawk 3 1 2 4 1 1 3 1 Common Raven 19 13 5 6 8 7 7 17 6 4 4 Loggerhead Shrike 1 2 23 92412 Turkey Vulture 1 Burrowing Owl 2 11 Unknown Buteo 1 2 Unknown Falcon 1 Unknown Raptor 2 TOTAL 40 28 16 22 15 25 33 27 41 26 33 28 Table 20. Monthly averages of birds of prey counted on the road transect in the Natural Area, October, 1973 - December, 1976. Species Jan Feb Mar Apr May Jun Jul Aug Sep Oct Nov Dec Golden Eagle 6.0 6.0 4.0 3.0 6.7 10.7 7.0 3.5 1.3 1.7 1.7 3.5 Prairie Falcon 2.7 4.0 8.3 7.4 2.0 2.3 5.5 1.0 0.7 1.0 4.3 2.3 Kestrel 5.7 6.0 4.3 2.7 3.7 4.0 10.5 5.0 11.7 3.8 3.0 7.8 Red-tailed Hawk 3.3 2.0 1.7 2.3 1.7 1.0 2.5 1.0 1.7 2.0 0.67 2.5 Marsh Hawk 3.0 2.0 1.3 2.3 1.7 1.3 + + 0.8 2.0 3.8 Common Raven 14.0 15.0 16.0 11.3 15.7 9.3 + 13.7 6.2 4.7 12.2 Loggerhead Shrike 1.7 1.0 0.7 1.3 12.0 6.0 2.0 2.8 1.7 1.8 Rough-legged Hawk 11.0 6.0 8.3 5.3 + 2.7 7.2 Ferruginous Hawk + Swainson's Hawk 1.3 0.7 + + Goshawk + Cooper's Hawk + + + 0.5 + Bald Eagle + Merlin + + Turkey Vulture + + Short-eared Owl fAsio flammeus) + + Burrowing Owl + 5.3 3.0 + + Barn Owl + Unknown Buteo + + + + + + Unknown Falcon Unknown Raptor + 1.3 + + 0.8 + Observed only in one transect. 42 and are rarely observed. Ferruginous hawks, Swainson's hawks (Buteo swainsoni) and turkey vultures are summer breeders but are rare or occupy habitats not sampled by the transects. Numbers of each species vary with arrival of winter or spring immigrants, courtship and brood rearing requirements, fledging of young, departure of summer emigrants and perhaps weather. Golden eagle numbers peak in June and decrease in July with dispersal. Dispersal of adults and young raptors during the hot periods of July results in low numbers observed from August until December. This period is typified by resident breeders and transient individ- uals. Wintering species begin to arrive in November and leave in March. The period when most birds are counted extends from January through March when breeding birds, winter residents and transients are present. These data do not completely show the relative numbers of each species in the BPSA because of the biases of the route. It did not sample all habitat types in proportion to their availability. In addition, it followed a powerline for more than 60 percent of its length and passed nests of five burrowing owls and the only Swainson's hawk in the BPSA. These transects will not be continued in 1977. Instead, seasonal abundance of each species and habitat utilized will be determined from observations made each month by Study I personnel. Golden Eagle Population Forty-one golden eagles were counted from aerial transects in January 1976, but only seven eagles were counted in October 1976. The January density (3.2 eagles/100 km2 or 8.2/100 mi2) and adult to immature ratio (73:23) were similar to previous years and have not changed appreciably since 1974. The October density (0.5 eagles/100 km2 or 1.4/100 mi ) was the lowest recorded since counts began in 1972 (Table 21). Also, as in October 1975, no immature eagles were observed on the survey. However, the sample size was too small to compute any valid age ratios. The 'Statistically significant decline in golden eagle numbers between 1972-73 and 1973-74 transects appeared to be related to a decline in the jackrabbit population and decreased golden eagle reproductive performance in the BPSA (Kochert et al, 1975). This does not appear to be the case with the October 1976 decline in numbers. The jackrabbit population appears to be increasing (Study IVA 1976 Annual Report) and golden eagle reproductive performance has not shown any significant decline since 1973. A 1976 range fire that destroyed 64,750 ha (250 mi ) of jackrabbit habitat in the transect area, conversion of large areas to cultiva- tion, weather and error may account for the decline. The matter will be further analysed. Table 21- Results of aerial transects on 17,920 km2 (7000 mi) of the Snake River floodplain, 1972-76. Golden Eagles Other No. No. No . Date Adults Immature Unknown Oct 72 10 11 8 Feb 73 33 33 IS Oct 73 7 3 7 Jan 74 20 9 12 Oct 74 4 2 10 Feb 75 17 S 7 Oct 75 10 0 5 Jan 76 24 9 6 Oct 76 4 0 3 Adult : Immature Eagle/100 m±2 Total ratio (256 km2) 29 43:52 34 50:50 17 70:30 41 69:31 16 67:33 32 63:32 15 100:00 41 73:27 7 ** 1.4 5 .3 6 .3 3 .4 8 2 3 2 6 4 0 o O 9 Other No. raptors* Coyotes 56 16 72 19 28 0 82 14 32 8 43 16 19 9 69 6 54 2 * includes bald eagles, red-tailed hawks, rough-legged hawks, ferruginous hawks, prairie falcons, American kestrels, marsh hawks, and unknown raptors. ** Adult: Immature ratio not calculated because of small sample size. 44 Management Experiments Shade construction. Heat prostration is a common cause of eaglet mortality in the study area (Kochert 1972, 1973; Kochert et al. 1975). In 1976, two golden eagle pairs nested on traditional sites with southwest exposures where young had previously died from heat prostration. At the control nest, a six week old eaglet was found dead below the nest on 2 June following several days of hot weather. At the other site, a single rock was placed on one side of the nest when the two occupant young were about two weeks old. The nest was visited every four days and the structure did not seem tall enough to provide mid-afternoon protection as the young exhibited signs of heat stress. When the young were about one month old, rocks were piled about 40 cm (16 inches) high in front and back of the nest. Plywood was laid across the top, weighted down, and partially covered with nest material. About half the nest surface was protected by the shade device. The eaglets could choose where to sit. On all nine subsequent visits, one or both young were under the shade. Both young fledged on approximately 15 June. The shade device did not cause any apparent change in food delivery by parents. An enclosed shade was constructed of rocks at an extremely late prairie falcon nest on 14 July where two young exhibited signs of heat stress. A single remaining young was inside the structure on both subsequent visits. By 3 August, prior to its expected fledging date, the young had disappeared from the scrape. At a ferruginous hawk nest that lacked shade, rocks were placed on one side to give some afternoon protection. The young were not observed using the shade. Two of the three original chicks and one of the two foster chicks (see below) fledged. There was no observed change in parental behavior. Fostering and moving of young. Five emaciated young ferruginous hawks were found at the Loveridge Site with a dead adult hawk nearby and one adult defending the area. The two youngest and most emaciated birds were taken to the ferruginous hawk nest where the shade device had been built (see above) . The remaining three young left in the nest fledged four and eight days later. The smallest of the fostered young was found dead below the nest where it had been placed four days earlier. The older of the fostered young fledged with the original two nestlings when the site was visited eight days after the introduction. Adult behavior did not change, and the foster nestlings were apparently accepted. At one of the few golden eagle nests located in farmland, the single ten day old nestling was missing on 13 May. The chick was alive on 9 May. Since the nest was vital to our eagle food habits study, a 3.5 week old eagle (the oldest from a nest with two young) was introduced on 15 May. The chick was hand fed on 45 introduction and again on 17 May when the foster adults had not returned. On 19 May, a foster adult eagle was with the young. We resumed four day interval food habits visits on 21 May. Seven subsequent visits were made until 21 June. The eaglet fledged on 27 June and was repeatedly seen in the vicinity of the nest until 11 August. The female was more attentive to this nest than most other female eagles in the BPSA, and attentiveness continued longer than typically observed. Four 3-week old prairie falcons were found below their nest. The scrape was highly infested with hemiptera parasites. We moved the young to the 1975 scrape, 100 m from the parasitized nest. Parasites did not become numerous in the new site. The adults continued to feed the young and defended the new site instead of the original 1976 site. All young fledged and used the 1975 site for resting. We observed no changes in the behavior of the adults. Disease treatment. In 1976, eaglets in food habits study nests were treated with Dimetridazole (Emtryl (R) , Salsbury Laboratories, Charles City, Iowa 50616). Emtryl was administered in gelatin capsules with 250 mg given to chicks less than one month and 500 mg to older chicks. In two of three cases where lesions were less than 10 mm^ when discovered, lesions disappeared after a single treatment. The third case required four treatments of 250 to 500 mg Emtryl administered every four days before symptoms disappeared. In two cases, a three week old chick had small lesions that increased in size after treatment began. One young died despite three treatments; lack of food may have been a contributing factor. The other young recovered after six treatments at four day intervals and showed no sign of new lesions in the following 16 days it was observed before fledging. A third serious case suddenly developed in a 7.5 week old eaglet between a four day visit interval. The emaciated bird was first treated with 500 mg. It was stronger four days later but still had large lesions. We again administered 500 mg Emtryl. Four days later, the bird's keel was within the breast muscle, but lesions had not decreased in size. We hand fed the chick until the crop was full and then gave it 750 mg Emtryl. The eaglet was alive and active 13 days later. On two subsequent visits, the eaglet was not observed in the area of the nest and is assumed to have fledged. In past years chicks with trichomoniasis were not treated. Birds with advanced cases were taken to a veterinarian, and these birds subsequently died. This year all but one treated bird lived to fledging age while most chicks with frounce in control nests died. 46 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS We wish to thank W. Mullins for field assistance. M. Collopy and J.R. Oakley analyzed some of the data presented in the report, and K. Steenhof helped the authors to prepare the final draft. We also acknowledge the cooperation of other study personnel in providing sightings of marked birds, locating nests, and assisting with field work. We are grateful to R. Dodge and P. Lewis for typing the final draft. LITERATURE CITED Baldwin, S.P. and S.C. Kendeigh. 1938. Variations in the weight of birds. Auk 55:416-467. Balph, D. 1973. Curlew Valley Validation Site Report: 1972 progress report. Research Memorandum. 73-1. Vol. 2:218-263. Beecham, J.J. 1970. Nesting ecology of the golden eagle in southwestern Idaho. M.S. Thesis. Univ. of Idaho, Moscow. 48 pp. Bellrose, F.C. 1976. Ducks, geese, and swans of North America. Stackpole Books. Harrisburg, PA. 543 pp. Bent, A.C. 1938. Life histories of North American Birds of Prey. Vol. I and II. U.S. Natl. Mus. Bull. 170, Washington, D.C. 409 and 482 pp. Boeker, E.L. and E.G. Bolen. 1972. Winter golden eagle populations in the southwest. J. Wildl. Manage. 36(2) :477-484. Brown, L.H. and A. Watson. 1964. The golden eagle in relation to its food supply. Ibis 106:78-100. Christensen, G.C. 1970. The chukar partridge: its introduction, life history and management. Nevada Dept. Fish and Game. Biol. Bull. No. 4. 82 pp. Craighead, J.J. and F.C. Craighead, Jr. 1956. Hawks, owls, and wildlife. Stackpole Co., Harrisburg, PA. and Wildl. Mgmt . Inst., Washington, D.C. 443 pp. Donohoe, R.W. 1966. Muskrat reproduction in areas of controlled and uncontrolled water-level units. J. Wildl. Manage. 30(2) :320-326. Esten, S.R. 1931. Bird weights of 52 species of birds (taken from notes of Wm. Van Gorder) . Auk 48:572-74. 47 Evans, F.C. and J.T. Emlen, Jr. 1947. Ecological notes on the prey selected by a barn owl. Condor 49(1) :3-9. Hall, E.R. 1946. Mammals of Nevada. Univ. Calif. Press, Berkeley. Hornocker, M.G., J. P. Messick, J.S. Whitman and G.W. Smith. 1976. Dynamics of predation upon a raptor prey base in the Snake Ri^er Birds of Prey Natural Area, Idaho in Snake River Birds of Prey Research Proj . Ann. Rep. 1976. U.S. Dept. of Interior, Bur. of Land Manage., Boise, ID. Howard, R.P. 1975. Breeding ecology of the ferruginous hawk in northern Utah and southern Idaho. M.S. Thesis. Utah State University, Logan. 60 pp. Johnson, D.R. and L. Diller. 1976. Ecology of reptiles in the Snake River Birds of Prey Natural Area in Snake River Birds of Prey Research Proj. Ann. Rep. 1976. U.S. Dept. of Interior, Bur. of Land Manage., Boise, ID. Johnson, D.R. and W.E. Melquist. 1975. Population ecology and habitat requirements of Townsend ground squirrels. Pages 156-178 in Snake River Birds of Prey Research Proj . Ann. Rep. 1975. U.S. Dept. of Interior, Bur. of Land Manage., Boise, ID. 193 pp. Kochert, M.N. 1972. Population status and chemical contamination in golden eagles in southwestern Idaho. M.S. Thesis. Univ. of Idaho, Moscow. 102 pp. . 1973. Density, food habits, and reproductive performance of raptors in the Snake River Birds of Prey Natural Area. Ann. Prog. Rep. Bur. of Land Manage. , Boise, ID. 54 pp. Kochert, M.N., R.P. Howard, and J. Silva. 1974. Reproductive performance, food habits and population dynamics of raptors in the Snake River Birds of Prey Natural Area. Ann. Prog. Rep., U.S. Dept. of Interior, Bur. of Land Manage., Boise, ID. 20 pp. Kochert, M.N., A.R. Bammann, R.P. Howard, J.H. Dor emus, M. Delate, and D. Donahue. 1975. Reproductive performance, food habits, and population dynamics of raptors in the Snake River Birds of Prey Natural Area. Pages 1-50 in Snake River Birds of Prey Research Proj. Ann. Rep. 1975. U.S. Dept. of the Interior, Bur. of Land Manage., Boise, ID. 193 pp. Kortright, F.H. 1942. Ducks, geese, and swans of North America. 3rd Ed. The Stackpole Co. Harrisburg, PA. and Wildl. Manage. Inst., Washington, D.C. 476 pp. McGahan, J. 1966. Biology of the golden eagle. M.S. Thesis. Univ. of Montana, Missoula. 78 pp. Messick, J. P., M.G. Hornocker, I.T. Mc T. Cowan and J. Whitman. 1975. Dynamics of predation upon a raptor prey base in the Snake River Birds of Prey Natural Area, Idaho. Pages 137-154 in Snake River Birds of Prey Research Proj . Ann. Rep. 1975. U.S. Dept. of Interior, Bur. of Land Manage., Boise, ID. 193 pp. Ogden, V.T. 1973. Nesting density and reproductive success of the prairie falcon (Falco mexicanus) in southwestern Idaho. M.S. Thesis. Univ. of Idaho, Moscow. 43 pp. Ogden, V.T. and M.G. Hornocker. 1977. Nesting density and success of prairie falcons in southwestern Idaho. J. Wildl. Manage. 41(1):1-11. Olendorff, R.R. 1973. The ecology of nesting birds of prey of northeastern Colorado. U.S.I.B.P. Tech. Rept . No. 211. 233 pp. Porter, R.D., and CM. White. 1973. The peregrine falcon in Utah, emphasizing ecology and competition with the prairie falcon. Brigham Young Univ. Sci. Bull. 18(1) :74 pp. Ratcliffe, D.A. 1962. Breeding density in the peregrine falcon (Falco peregrinus) and raven (Corvus corax) • Ibis 104:13-39. Smith, D.G., and J.R. Murphy. 1973. Breeding ecology of raptors in the eastern Great Basin of Utah. Brigham Young Univ. Sci. Bull. Biol. Ser. 18(3):l-76. Wolfe, M. and J.R. Montan. 1975. Density and species composition of the major raptor prey species in relation to land use and habitat type. Pages 98-126 in Snake River Birds of Prey Research Proj. Ann. Rep. 1975, U.S. Dept. of Interior, Bur. of Land Manage., Boise, ID. 193 pp. 49 Appendix 1. Weight of prey items found in raptor nests, Snake River Birds of Prey Study Area, 1976. Approximate weight, grams Species (Larg MAMMALS Black-tailed jackrabbit (Lepus calif- ornicus) Male 1878 Female 2342 Unknown 2110 Mountain cotton- tail (Sylvilagus nuttalli) 827 Townsend ground squirrel (Spermophilus townsendi) male 260 female 187 unknown 211 Whitetailed antelope squirrel (Ammo spermophilus leucurus) 106 Adult Medium Juvenile Unknown (Large) (Intermediate) (Small) (Average) Source 1557 500 471 215 1379 514 167 106 161 Stoddard (Pers. Comm.) This Study, Wolfe & Montan (1975) Johnson & Melquist (1975) Hall (1946) Least chipmunk (Eutamius minimus) 78 Smith & Murphy (1973) Yellowbellied marmot (Marmot a flavi- ventris) 3000 1500 500 1667 Hall (1946) Townsend pocket gopher (Thomomys town- sendii) 240 200 Hall (1946) This Study 50 Species Valley pocket gopher (Thomomys bottae) 170 Pocket gopher unidentified (Thomomys spp.) Harvest Mouse (Reithrodontotnys megalotis) Great Basin pocket mouse (Perognathus parvus) 15 Great Basin Kangaroo rat (Dipodomys microps) Kangaroo rat, Unidentified (Dipodomys spp.) 55.5 Deer mouse (Peromyscus mani- culatus) 17 Meadow mouse (Microtus spp.) 60 Adult Medium Juvenile Unknown (Large) (Intermediate) (Small) (Average) Source House mouse (Mus musculus) Bushy-tailed wood rat (Neotoma cinerea) Desert woodrat (Neotoma lepida) Woodrat, Unidentified (Neotoma spp.) 205 15 65 lr. 287 118 203 Smith & Murphy (1973) This Study Evans & Emlen (1947) Smith & Murphy (1973) Smith & Murphy (1973) Hall (1946) Smith & Murphy (1973) Porter & White (1973) Evans & Emlen (1947) Wolfe & Montan (1975) Wolfe & Montan (1975) This Study 51 Species muskrat (Ondatra zibeth- Ica) Mouse, Unidentified (Unknown) Shrew, Unidentified (Sorex spp. ) Adult Medium Juvenile Unknown (Large) (Intermediate) (Small) (Average) 1261 1052 1157 20 18 Source Donohoe (1966) This Study This Study Domestic cat (Fells domesticus) Long-tailed weasel (Mustela f ren- ata) Coyote (Canis latrans) Badger (Taxidea taxus) 178 2043 2392 This 1800 Study Smith & Murphy (1973) This Study Hornocker et al . (1976) Mule deer (Odocoileus hemionus) BIRDS 6300 McGahan (1966) Gray partridge (Perdix perdix) 376 Ring-necked pheasant (Phasianus colchicus) male 1380 female 1017 unknown 1199 California quail (Lophortyx californicus) Chukar (Alectoris graeca) 1093 198 571 McGahan (1966) Wolfe & Montan (1975) Porter & White (1973) Christ- ensen (1970) 52 Species Adult Medium Juvenile Unknown (Large) (Intermediate) (Small) (Average) Source Common Raven (Corvus corax) Black-billed magpie (Pica pica) 173 Rock dove (Columba livia) 330 Mourning dove (Zenaida macr' ira) Yellow warbler (Dendroica petechia) Starling (Sturnus vulgaris) Says phoebe (Sayornis saya) Brewer's black- bird (Euphagus cyanocephalus) Red-winged black- bird (Agelaius phoeni- ceus) 54 Western meadowlark (Sturnella neglecta) 96 Horned lark (Eremophila alpestris) 28 Loggerhead shrike (Lanius ludovic- ianus) 871 300 113 20 84 21 68 52 This Study Smith & Murphy (1973) This Study Balph (1973) This Study Smith & Murphy (1973) Porter White (1973) Porter & White (1973) Porter & White (1973) Wolfe & Montan (1975) Wolfe & Montan (1975) Esten (1931) 5 3 Species Pintail (Anas acuta) Mallard (Anus platyrhynchos) male 1219 female 1077 unknown 1148 Adult Medium Juvenile Unknown jLarge) (Intermediate) (Small) (Average) Source Unknown duck Killdeer (Charadrius vocif erus) Great blue heron (Ardea herodias) Ring-billed gull (Larus delawarensis) Gull, Unidentified (Larus spp . ) Rock Wren (Salpinctes obsoletus) White-crowned sparrow (Zonotrichia leucophrys) Sage sparrow (Amphispiza belli) Sparrow, Unidentified (Unknown) 697 150 982 106 2807 419 504 13 30 30 17 Bellrose (1976) Kortright (1942) Kortright (1942) Porter & White (1973) Porter & White (1973) Esten (1931) Harris (Pers. Comm.) Harris (Pers. Comm.) Harris (Pers. Comm.) Baldwin & Kendeigh (1938) This Study Balph ,1973) Prairie falcon (Falco mexicanus) 715 This Study 54 Species Adult Medium Juvenile Unknown (Large) (Intermediate) (Small) (Average) Source Red-tailed hawk (Buteo jamaicen- sis) male 1028 female 1224 unknown 1126 Great horned owl (Bubo virginianus) 1505 Ferruginous hawk (Buteo regalis) 1056 1230 1254 Craighead & Craighead (1956) This 1091 Study Craighead & Craighead (1956) 1368 This Study Howard (1975) REPTILES AND AMPHIBIANS Side-blotched lizard (Uta stansburiana) Johnson & Diller (1976) Western fence lizard (Sceloporus occi- dentalis 18 17 Johnson & Diller (1976) Desert horned lizard (Phrynosoma platy- rhinos) 24 11 18 Johnson & Diller (1976) Western whiptail lizard (Cnemidophorus tigris 17 15 Johnson & Diller (1976) Collared lizard (Crotaphytus collaris) 35 26 Johnson & Diller (1976) Leopard lizard (Crotaphytus wislizenii) 26 11 23 Johnson & Diller (1976) Lizard, Unidentified (Unknown) 24 17 Johnson & Diller (1976) 5S Species Woodhouse's toad (Bufo woodhousei) Toad, Unidentified (Unknown) Gopher snake (Pituophis melano- leucus) 273 Western rattlesnake (Crotalus viridis) 622 Ground snake (Sonora semiannulata) Long-nosed snake (Rhinocheilus lecontei) Striped whipsnake (Masticophis taenia- tus) 101 Night snake (Hypsiglena torquata) Racer (Coluber constrictor) Unknown snake (Unknown ) 332 FISH Adult Medium Juvenile Unknown (Large) (Intermediate) (Small) (Average) Source 147 305 19 60 20 :•■() 20 146 329 184 33 51 97 14 68 184 Diller (Pers. Comm.) Diller (Pers. Comm.) Johnson & Diller (1976) Johnson & Diller (1976) Johnson & Diller (1976) Johnson Si Diller (1976) Johnson & Diller (1976) Johnson & Diller (1976) Johnson & Diller (1976) Johnson & Diller (1976) Carp 'Cyprinus carpio) 500 This Study lb Adult Medium Juvenile Unknown Species (Large) (Intermediate) (Small) (Average) Source INVERTEBRATES Grasshopper (Locustidae) 0.63 Scarab beetle (Scarabidae) 0 . 30 Scorpion (Scorpionidae) 1.45 Cicada (Cicadidae) 1 . 0 Jerusalem Cricket (Gryllacrididae) 2 . 0 Wasp (Hymenoptera) 0 . 5 Smith & Murphy(1973) Smith & Murphy(1973) Smith & Murphy (1973) This Study Evans & Emlen (1947) This Study 57 Notes 58 STUDY II: CONTRACTOR: INVESTIGATORS : PROJECT SUPPORT: OBJECTIVES: Responses of nesting raptors to human disturbance in the Snake River Birds of Prey Natural Area, Idaho. University of Idaho, Moscow, Idaho 83843. Steven R. Peterson, Principal Investigator. Norman Stewart, Research Assistant. U.S. Department of the Interior, Bureau of Land Management, Contract No. YA-512-CT6-69. 1. Identify human activities that affect the reproductive success of raptors in the Birds of Prey Natural Area (BPNA) . a. Test specific hypotheses concerning these disturbances to prairie falcons in relation to proximity of nest, time of day, and point in the reproductive cycle. b. Note the responses of other raptors to human disturbances and possibly correlate these with reproductive success. 2. Measure prairie falcon habituation to initially disturbing events. 3. If prairie falcons can be habituated to specific disturbances, suggest ways of exploiting the phenomena for use in research and visitor development. ANNUAL SUMMARY This study was terminated during the fall due to lack of funds. Reinitiation will depend upon adequate funding after baseline data on undisturbed raptor behavior has been collected. Because of initial difficulties in establishing a study area where our activities did not conflict with other research on raptors in the BPNA, data collection was minimal this first summer. However, we have summarized our general impressions and have suggested some additional avenues of research, if the project should be refunded. METHODS Most of our time was spent in observation, often in conjunction with other researchers working in the study area, particularly Study I. Most of our data pertaining to the establishment of breeding territories were gathered in the Swan Falls Dam area. 59 When possible, research activities were coordinated so that observations on the behavior of nesting raptors could be made before, during and after visits to a nest site by an investigator. Observations were also made on the response of raptors to boating activity, both public boating and trips by BPNA research personnel . At the end of the breeding season, the research assistant aided John Doremus in measuring the physical attributes of the nesting sites. RESULTS Our observations do not indicate whether breeding abandonment is the result of a single event or an accumulation of pressures. Observations on prairie falcons (Falco mexicanus) suggest that human activity well below a nest site resulted in little or no response from the falcons. This was observed in river narrows as well as locations inland. If the prairie falcon does fly off the cliff, it lands soon after. A sudden loud noise such as a dropped rock or gun report, will often cause a bird to fly off the cliff, but once again it lands almost immediately. Human presence above the scrape, while not always resulting in flight of the birds due to less visual or auditory contact, will keep prairie falcons in the air until the humans depart, if the falcons were disturbed. Prairie falcons are more difficult to force (bump) on the nest site when eggs are in a late stage of incubation or when young chicks are present. Human activity in relation to parent prairie falcons appears less important late in the nesting season because the adults are often not at the nest site during much of the day, whereas considerable human activity early in the season, especially when territories are being established, increases the probability of site abandonment. Among larger raptors, particularly golden eagles (Aquila chrysaetos) , that frequently fly out of view after being forced from a nest site, the time away from the nest can vary greatly and seems to have little correlation with site abandonment. Golden eagles have not returned for several hours only to be seen active near the nest several days later, while others have returned in less than one-half hour and the nest was later found abandoned. Time was not available to observe these nests intensively, and human contacts that we are unaware of may have occurrred after the initial observation. Our observations suggest that considerable variability occurs within the golden eagle population utilizing the BPNA in regards to their tolerance of humans. A nesting site readily observed and easily accessible to humans will usually fail. Generally renesting attempts in the 60 same vicinity will also fail. The latter failures may be due to a number of causes, such as the presence of humans, high cliff face temperatures and resultant over-heating of eggs when adults are absent from the nest, or decreases in available prey. SUGGESTIONS FOR FUTURE RESEARCH Before any additional research is undertaken concerning the effects of humans on nesting raptor populations, the term "disturbance" in reference to humans, should be more precisely defined in the context it will be investigated. It is unrealistic to assume that when a human comes within sight of a raptor and no visual response is elicited, that the raptor is not necessarily "disturbed." The visual response may actually be the end-point triggered by a series of events up to that time. A controlled study utilizing pen-reared birds equipped with cardiac transmitters (this method has been used successfully on ground squirrels) would suggest when raptors are stimulated by humans under specific conditions. On the other hand, what we perceive as a disturbance may not actually disturb the specific raptor at all. For example, prairie falcons emit little or no visible response to human activities below a nest site, especially late in the breeding season. Just because humans are within close visual contact with a raptor does not necessarily mean the events are disturbing to that specific raptor. It is the opinion of this investigator that a study designed to investigate human disturbances to a raptor population should really be examining the entire entity, i.e. behavior and breeding success, with human activity as just one important variable. This approach would allow human activity to be examined in proper perspective. Different human-raptor contacts would have to be identified including previous exposure (reflected in land-use patterns of the area) and present exposure as reflected by public visits and research activity. The disturbance threshold, as well as the intensity of disturbances, would have to be quantified and ultimately related back to breeding success in specific areas. An inherent problem in this type of study would be individual variation within the raptor population. Data from the BPNA suggests, at least in golden eagles, that some individuals are more tolerant of human activity than others. Consequently, for the data to have much meaning, a fairly large sample size would be necessary, such as could be obtained with prairie falcons in the BPNA. It would be unwise to conduct an in-depth study of this type on species such as the ferruginous hawk (Buteo ragalis) , simply because an insufficient breeding population exists in the study area. In this type of study, it will be necessary to cause some nest site abandonment by repeated human-raptor contacts, both prior to and during egg laying. If this is not accomplished, 61 then the disturbance threshold for specific periods of the breeding cycle will remain unknown. This aspect of the research will be unpopular with various "raptor-loving" groups, but I believe it will be necessary for the data to have validity. A study site(s) will have to be secured in which all human activity, investigative as well as recreational, will be controlled. Lastly, 1 believe the time for this type of study is long overdue. We are in an era of resource management in which humans have a potentially dominant influence on that resource. In areas such as the BPNA, humans are going to demand more opportunities for recreation. Unless managers know what the effects human activities have on that raptor population, the reproductive effort of the raptors may decline, or people may be unjustly deprived of enjoying a public resource to its fullest. 62 Thomas Dunstan, Study III, affixes a radio transmitter to one of six golden eagles tracked by research personnel in 1976 (BLM photo by Dunstan) . 63 STUDY III: CONTRACTOR: INVESTIGATORS : PROJECT SUPPORT: Activity, hunting patterns, territoriality, and social interactions of birds of prey in the Snake River Birds of Prey Natural Area, Idaho. Department of Biological Sciences Western Illinois University Thomas C. Dunstan, Principal Investigator. James F. Harper, Nat. Sci. Tech. Assistant. Kenneth B. Phipps, Research Assistant. U.S. Department of the Interior, Bureau of Land Management Contract No. 52500-CT5-1013. OBJECTIVES: 2. To determine the space needed by breeding golden eagles, prairie falcons, and red-tailed hawks for food gathering within the Snake River Birds of Prey Natural Area. To determine the daily activity rhythms of each of the species and to ascertain the inter-relationships between the three species. 3. To determine the fledging and dispersal activities of the young of the three species. 4. To coordinate the above mentioned objectives with the objectives of investigators studying the ecology of the prey species of the three species of raptors previously mentioned. ANNUAL SUMMARY The habitat use and behavior of golden eagles (Aquila chrysaetos) at six nest sites, of red-tailed hawks (Buteo jamaicensis) at five nest sites, and prairie falcons (Falco mexicanus) at eight nest sites were studied in 1976 as part of a three year study. Radio transmitters were placed on 2 adult and 3 young golden eagles, 1 adult and 2 young red-tailed hawks, and 9 adult and 10 young prairie falcons. Horizontal space use, hunting behavior, daily activities and rhythms, territoriality, behavior of the young, dispersal, and interactions were studied. PLANS FOR NEXT SEASON To continue to monitor behavior and activities of all three species in five study areas, including an eastern extension outside the BPNA. 64 2. To capture and place radios on selected individuals with primary emphasis on gathering quantitative data about hunting behavior as related to habitat use. 3. To coordinate efforts of Study III personnel with the personnel from other studies. 4. To analyze, compile, and evaluate data in the preparation of the Final Report. 5 . To compare and discuss the relationship of the data from Study III with other studies in the pertinent literature. STUDY AREA Raptors were studied at four sites within the Snake River Birds of Prey Natural Area (BPNA) , Idaho. The designated sites were named: Western, Intensive, Mid-desert, and Eastern sites (Fig. 1). METHODS Definition of Terms The following are definitions of terms used in this study. "Territory" is a defended area. "Total range" includes all of the horizontal space used by study subjects when we monitored. For this report, "home range" includes all area used except areas used on only one flight, and areas used after the young dispersed. "Sunrise" and "sunset" refer to official sunrise and sunset respectively. "Hours monitored" is the length of time that the location of a marked bird was known. A "spot check" was a random monitoring of a bird's location, usually for 15 to 30 minutes as we passed in the vicinity. During a "sample day" we monitored for more than several hours, usually for the entire day. Two or three consecutive sample days constituted a "sample period." A "perch site" was a rocky ledge, power pole, cairn, or other object a bird repeatedly used as a perch when hunting, loafing, or roosting. A "trap day" was a day when trapping was attempted. "Man-hours" was the time spent setting up a trapping apparatus and waiting nearby for a capture. "Set-hours" included only the time that the trap was set to capture a subject. Frequently we monitored locations of several birds simultaneously. If a second bird flew in a different direction while we were tracking a subject, we only noted a flight was made, since we were not able to plot the second bird's flight path. This distinction is referred to as "flights followed/flights noted." Home Nest Ran%e Species D O A Fig. 1. Location of 20 nest sites and 14 home ranges of birds during 1976. Adjacent prairie falcon ranges have been combined. The 4 Study Sites are indicated in parentheses . 66 Trapping We used the same trapping techniques as in 1975 (Dunstan and Harper 1975). In 1976, an additional technique, a drop net, was used. A 1.8 x 1.8 m piece of gill net (6.3 cm mesh), reinforced peripherally with 12-gauge plastic covered wire, was designed to be dropped at night and entangle a bird perched below. Instrumentation Captured raptors were weighed, measured, and sexed. Numbered U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service metal leg bands (pop rivet for eagles, lock-tite for hawks) were applied, and an identification code was assigned to each bird (Table 1) . Radio packages as described by Dunstan and Harper (1975) were used to mark study subjects. Some wing markers were also placed on subjects by personnel from Study I (see this report, Study I). In 1976, two radio packages used on the adult eagles contained two transmitters within a green back sack. Each package weighed 180 g. Monitoring Instrumented Subjects Radio-tagged birds were tracked from ground vehicles, aircraft, and on foot, using receivers and antennas previously described (Dunstan and Harper 1975). In 1976, a second tracking truck, a Dodge pickup, was used in addition to the International Travelall used in 1975. On sample days, we continuously monitored the position of marked birds from 0.5 hours before sunrise to 0.5 hour after sunset. Due to vehicle breakdowns and the difficulty of simultaneously following two to four birds in different directions, there were periods of up to several hours in some sample days when no data was gathered from certain individuals. Prairie falcons in the Intensive Study Site were monitored three consecutive days in every 11-day period. Falcons in the Eastern Study Site were sampled on two consecutive days per week. The remaining study subjects were tracked for one, two, or three consecutive days at irregular intervals. Information gathered during spot checks and trapping days added to the total hours monitored. Monitoring Non-Instrumented Birds Unmarked members of families were also studied. Individuals were identified by plumage characteristics (color variations, various stages of molt) , and association with a nesting site or other family members. Size dimorphism in golden eagles was distinguishable, and eagles regularly used favored perches and hunting areas. Because of their large size, eagles were seen and followed to and from eyries. Red-tailed hawks were also large enough to be followed visually. 67 Table 1. Data for 31 marked study subjects, 1976 I.D. Band Instru- Location Code Sex Age Number mented Dispersed GOLDEN EAGLE Cabin Draw CD U Yg 629-02605 19 July 26 Oct. Cabin Draw CD u Yg 629-02606 — — Rapids (RD) GE-1 u Yg 599-11928a 15 June '75 6-11 Feb. Feedlot FL F Ad 629-02602 12 May (remained) Feedlot FL u Yg 629-026303 — 1-10 Aug. Feedlot FL u Yg 629-02631 19 June 17-20 July Black Butte BB F Ad 629-02603 3 July 8-9 July Black Butte BB U Yg 629-026353 3 July 8-9 Julyc RED-TAILED HAWK Priest Ranch RT-1 M Ad 987-16303 29 May '75 13-17 Aug. Black Butte B M Ad 987-16304 13 June 1-7 Sept. Black Butte B U Yg 987-16305 11 July 17-19 Aug. Spoon S U Yg 987-21 968a 7 July 5-19 Aug. PRAIRIE FALCON Spring s2 F Ad 877-37802 14 May 13 July New No* M Ad 836-77002, 987-16360° 987-21951° 27 March 21 July Priest P-l U Yg 17 June 23 July Priest P-2 u Yg 17 June 19-20 July PF-2 PF-2? F Ad 987-16302 27 March 28 June Camera §f M Ad 836-77006 . 967-57231° 987-16336° 987-16354° 987-21955° 987-21954° 30 April 10 July Camera F Ad 3 April 12 July Camera C-l U Yg 12 June 1-2 July Camera C-2 U Yg 16 June 7-8 July Finger F u Yg 20 June 15-16 Julyc Powerline W u Yg 17 June 14 July Upper Bigfoot D-4 U Yg 987-219583 17 June 16 July Upper Bigfoot D-7 U Yg 786-94379a 17 June 15-16 July Loaf If M Ad 836-77004, 967-57288° 23 April 4-6 July Loaf F Ad 23 April 21-23 Junec Humpback 5? H Ad 836-77003 12 April mid-October Humpback F Ad 877-37801 21 April 2 July Humpback H-ll U Yg 987-2191 la 10 June 3-6 July Humpback H-12 u Yg 987-219123 12 June 29 June- 1 July a. Banded by Study I personnel. b. Banded and instrumented by Study VII personnel. c. Died d. Banded by V. Ogden, Idaho Coop. Wildlife Res. Unit, 1972. 68 Data Analysis Golden eagle. To derive range maps of horizontal space use, all sightings of an adult pair during the breeding season were combined. Mated eagles regularly used the same perches and often perched and flew together over the same areas. Although one of the sexes used a specific location more than the other, both used the same areas and therefore had identical ranges. At the feedlot site, we monitored for full days and noted activity (perched versus flying, and location). The data were obtained by noting the activity and location of the subject every 10 minutes. Prairie falcon. To clarify the increases in falcon range size as the breeding season progressed, four ''periods" were designated: incubation, nestling, fledgling, and dispersal. These divisions were based on changes in the adult behavior, mainly a noticeable increase in the distance of the furthest flight monitored during a two or three day sample. Due to the nature of our sampling schedule (three days "on" with eight days "off" at the Intensive Study Site, and two days "on" with five days "off" at the Eastern Site) , cutoff dates between the four periods were not abrupt. The four divisions also were closely related to the development of the young. During the "incubation" period, adult females rarely left the canyon. Approximately a week after the young hatched, adult females began making several flights per day out of the canyon, and this change in behavior marks the beginning of the "nestling" (brood-rearing) period. The "fledgling7' (post-fledgling) period began within a week after the young fledged, when the adults began making flights distinctly further from the canyon than previously monitored. The "dispersal" period included increases in space used after fledglings dispersed from the BPNA, and just prior to departure of the adults. Not all adults exhibited definite increases in cumulative range with each change in period. The four divisions demonstrate the changing nature of the horizontal space used as the breeding season progressed. In analysing daily rhythms, we measured the portion of the diurnal day spent out of the canyon (minimum of 100 m from the rim). The day was divided into half hour increments, and the amount of time the adult bird spent out of the canyon was divided by the amount of time we monitored the bird. For example, between 0900 and 0930 hours, we were monitoring on eight days (a total of four hours), and the bird spent a total of 1.5 hours out of the canyon on those eight days. The portion of time spent out of the canyon is a decimal of 0.38 (1.5 hrs/4 hrs) . The probability of finding the falcon out of the canyon during that particular half hour on any given day would be 38 percent. By totaling the decimal value for each half hour increment, and then dividing by the number of half hour divisions in the diurnal day, the average time 69 spent out of the canyon is derived. By grouping the monitoring days into the four developmental periods, we determined values for the average time spent out of the canyon for each bird for each period. Similarly, we derived the average number of round trips north of the canyon per day per bird. For every half hour increment in a group of sample days, the numbers of round trips was divided by the number of days monitored during that half hour. The total of all the half hourly decimal values in a diurnal day is the average number of round trips per day for that developmental period. For example, during a developmental period, we may have been in a position to monitor on 10 days between 0800-0830 hours, but could only monitor on seven days between 2000-2030 hours. By deriving a decimal value for each half hour increment, we minimized the effects of data gaps in our full day sampling. Flight speeds were determined by dividing the distance between two known points by the time interval it took the bird to travel that distance. Red-tailed hawks. The methods of monitoring and data analysis for red-tailed hawks were the same as for eagles and falcons. RESULTS Golden Eagle Chronology. Eagles were the first of the three species to initiate breeding activities (Fig. 2) . Nest repair began in January and continued into February at the Rapids, Ogden, and Beecham nests in the Intensive and Western Study Sites. We planned to work with the Rapids (RD) eagles a second year, but after the adults repaired the nest and courted, they laid no eggs. The Ogden (OG) pair nested on the steep north canyon wall, which was inaccessible for study. The Beecham pair repaired nests, but did not breed in 1976. The eagles at Black Butte (BB) and Feedlot (FL) eyries had a similar chronology, but the Cabin Draw (CD) pair nested about a month later. During incubation, field personnel remained away from the nest sites to minimize disturbance. Nestlings remained in the eyries into June and July, and fledging occurred at an age of 8-11 weeks. One radio-tagged fledgling at the Feedlot site dispersed in mid-July, GE-1 from the Rapids eyrie in 1975 did not disperse until February 1976. Adult golden eagles remained within the breeding range for the entire year. Nest Site 1 R CD 3 5 BB O = = FFF d--dDD = = = FFFF-- D ■d-d ■-D LLLL: :HHH = = FFF ■. . . . .D eggs in nest young in nest young flying no young remain Fig. 2. Chronology of the 1976 breeding season. Key L egg laying H hatching F fled Ring D adult dispersed d young dispersed X adult found dead x young found dead O 71 Trapping effort. A total of 207 man-hours and 147 set-hours on 20 trapping days were expended to capture two adult golden eagles. Approximately half this effort was spent in the Intensive Study Site in February and March. The other half was spent in the Mid-Desert and Eastern areas in June and July. Horizontal space use. Movement and perch site data were gathered from the Rapids, Ogden, Beecham, and Swan Falls Dam eagles by direct observation from 2 January to 31 March, 1976. During observations on 26 days (including census days for pole use), 59 golden eagle sightings were noted perched on crossbars of the tall power line (138 KV) . Five sections between poles numbered 195 through 278 were preferred (Fig. 3). The flight paths of the Ogden and Rapids eagles, as well as 46 additional golden eagle sightings away from the power line were noted. The clumping of locations of certain sightings, when combined with flight path data, indicate a relationship to specific nest sites. Within the Intensive Study Site the tall pole line and the buttes were the favored loafing and hunting perches. Eagles of various age classes perched on top of or soared over Coyote Butte, which protrudes 60 m above the desert flats. Raptors seemingly were attracted to the butte because of its shape, which caused windward updrafts, lee waves, and slope drafts suitable for soaring. The same situation, but to a lesser degree, existed at Trio Butte, which protrudes 30 m above the desert floor. During the winter of 1975-76, we also made eight bald eagle sightings. The young eagle (GE-1) from the 1975 Rapids nest remained within its home range until 6 to 12 February when it dispersed. Breeding season horizontal space use information was collected from the Cabin Draw, Black Butte, and Feedlot pairs. Neither Cabin Draw (CD) adult was radio-tagged, and on the basis of 89 pairs of visual observations, the minimum home range of the pair was 12 km^ . The birds used both the north and south sides of the Snake River (Fig. 4). Favored perches near the eyrie were north across the ravine along the rock cliff, and on the point 60 m west of the nest. These perches were used at night for roosting, and occasionally during the day when the young were still in the nest. Other favored perches were located along the rim south of the eyrie. Perches on the shaded north and west sides of Jackass Butte were used often during the hot summer days. Mid-day thermal and slope updrafts were used for soaring at Jackass Butte, and adult eagles hunted along the rims and slopes. Additional favored perch sites were on vertical uprights and horizontal crossbars of the tall power line 1.5 km north of the eyrie. Both the male and female favored pole 241 (short power line) , located close to a draw with relatively tall vegetation. They also hunted from pole 238 on the flats. Poles 105 to 112 of the tall power line received use as perch sites, as did several cairns and rock piles. 72 A 1976 nest A 1975 nest :;;; Farm • Sighting ■ Flight path Favored section o f power 1 ine / c \ No . sightings / section of poles Total range of GE- 1 (21 km2 from 6/75-2/76) |. 1 Mi -—•-Minimum total range of Rapids adults (29 km2) 1 Km Fig. 3. Golden eagle sightings away from the nesting areas from 2 January to 31 March, 1976. There were 59 sightings along 5 favored sections of the tall (138 KV) power line, and 45 other sightings in random sampling. 73 Site Area Hours Days Cabin Draw 12 km2 89 20 Black Butte 21 km2 28 15 Samp le period 9 June- 1 3 June 19 No -27 0 v , ct 1 Km Jk Nest • : • Farm • Perch Fig. 4. Area used by adult golden eagles at the Cabin Draw (CD) and Black Butte (BB) sites, 1976. 74 The Black Butte (BB) adults frequented both sides of the Snake River, covering at least 21 km2, determined by 28 hours of monitoring (Fig. 4). The adult female was instrumented on 3 July, but disappeared on 8 or 9 July after the death of her fledgling. Favored perches near the eyrie were 500 m southwest along and just below the rim. Two favored night roosts were used during the breeding and post-breeding seasons. They were 15 and 12 m below the rim and 150 and 375 m northeast of the eyrie respectively. Other perches in sight of, but further from, the eyrie were at the apex and west slope of Black Butte, and on the east end of Jackass Butte. Birds perched on the east end of Jackass Butte could not see Cabin Draw eagles perched on the west end of Jackass Butte. Perches out of sight of the eyrie were along the short pole line between poles 265-268, along the tall pole line between poles 88- 90, and on several cairns further north. From the top, or on the slopes of Black Butte (which rises 115 m above the desert flats), the adult eagles could see almost all of their home range, as well as a majority of the home range of the Cabin Draw eagles to the west, and the Beer Case pair of eagles to the east. The large surface area of Black Butte caused slope drafts suitable for soaring. Updrafts were also common along the ravines and draws in Section 11 west and northwest of the butte, and this area was often used for hunting. During the breeding season, the Feedlot (FL) adults used a home range of 48 km2 (Fig. 5). No flights south of the Snake River 4.5 km from the nest were noted. Between the nest and the river, an intensively cultivated flood plain provided limited habitat for Townsend ground squirrels (Spermophilus townsendi) and black-tailed jackrabbits (Lepus californicus) , the prey most often brought to the nest. At this site, the adult female and one of two nestlings were instrumented. The adult female was identified to be an eagle released as a one year old in 1964 by Morlan Nelson (Morlan Nelson, pers. comra. ) . Favored perches near the eyrie were 15 m to the south along and just below the rim. The adults also used a rock point 150 m directly across from the eyrie, and perched along the draws in Section 22 west of the eyrie. Shaded perches below the rim were used on hot summer afternoons. Preferred crossbars along the short power line were on pole 347 (on top of a hill over looking the ravines close to the eyrie) , pole 357 (near a creek bed) , and pole 361. Preferred perches along the tall power line were at poles 54, 48, and 33. Dorsey Butte and adjacent ridge were often used for perching. Dorsey Butte rises 60 m above the desert flats and provides a vantage point with line of sight to all areas of the home range except for below the rim. Additional favored perch sites were near dry creek beds or rocky areas, which provided the tallest 75 Home range Area Hours Days Sample period Breeding season 48 km2 249 62 10 May- 29 Jan ( 7 7 ) Post-breeding 18 ki 10 2 9 16 Nov-29 Jan(77) m. Nest • • ■ Farm o Perch Fig. 5. Area used by adult golden eagles at the Feed lot (FL) site, 1976. 76 vegetation in the area, and habitat for favored prey, especially black-tailed jackrabbits and desert cottontails (Sylvilagus nuttalli) . Between 24 June and 25 July, the Feedlot adult female made four trips 60 to 70 km northeast to the foothills north of Glenns Ferry and King Hill. On each occasion, the eagle remained in the foothills overnight, and in one case, was absent from the home range for at least 4.5 days (6-10 July). When monitored on three departure and two return flights, the initiation of flight occurred between 1230 and 1705 hours. In all cases, trips were made without landing along the way, and the eagle remained airborne a maximum of 3.5 hours. The radio-tagged fledgling dispersed between 17-20 July (when the adult female was within the home range) . The Feedlot adults used a 18 km^ post-breeding range as determined by sampling between 16 November 1976 to 29 January 1977 (Fig. 4). The wintering eagles were monitored on full day samples from 45 minutes before sunrise to 45 minutes after sunset. The bird's locations were grouped into three geographic areas: 1) the eyrie, including the draw just west, and the nearby small pole line, 2) the rim and slopes southeast of the nest, and 3) north of the eyrie including the tall power lines and Dorsey Butte. These areas were the three general areas that the wintering birds seemed to prefer for hunting. Other large areas within and adjacent to these were not used. Our purpose in analyzing the data in this way was to determine relative use of each of the three general areas as they relate to hunting use. The three areas were identified as Eyrie, South, and North respectively. The marked adult female was monitored for 100 hours on nine sample days, and spent 21 percent, 12 percent, and 67 percent of the time in the Eyrie, South, and North areas respectively. The adult male (not instrumented) was monitored for 45 hours on eight sample days and spent 1 percent, 2 percent, and 97 percent of the time in the Eyrie, South, and North areas, respectively. The male and female were observed simultaneously, but the male occasionally was lost from sight when it flew as we watched the female. The male was sometimes not seen for several hours. This accounts for the difference in hours monitored. The female repaired the 1976 eyrie, and also started a new nest in January 1977. Sticks and grasses were gathered from nearby, and this accounts for the greater percentage of time spent near the eyrie. Boundaries for the post-breeding range were related to the location of favored perches, which were related to hunting methods used in taking favored prey (probably jackrabbits) during this season. 77 Hunting behavior. Data on hunting were gathered by direct observation and radio-tracking. All of the eagles studied used several methods of hunting. The four methods of hunting seemed to be related to the physical conditions for various types of flight, the preferred prey, and the prey habitat. Eagles hunted while watching from perches on cairns, rims, and power line crossbars. Flights from perches were either direct to the prey, or were out over habitat in search of prey and then back to the perch. In the latter case eagles attempted to flush prey or surprise it during the flights. Eagles also hunted by watching for prey during long (time and distance) flights. Flapping or soaring flights were of several types. Flapping flights were common in morning, late afternoon, and evening until after sunset. During flapping flights, eagles flew rapidly just above the vegetation and crossed back and forth over several square kilometers. Soaring flights occurred during late morning, mid-day, and early afternoon at elevations of approximately 20 to 500 m. To travel distances more than a kilometer, eagles used downward glides that terminated by spiraling upward. Hunting from soaring flights was not common. A third method of hunting, commonly used from mid-day until early evening, was a combination of flapping and soaring along rocky areas with slope updrafts. Eagles slowly flew over and along the rims and talus slopes with a hunting style typical of a harrier (Circus cyaneus). These flights were made where there were boulders and vegetation that appeared to be good habitat for cottontails . A fourth method of hunting flight was a combination of flapping flight and short glides. This method was commonly used when hunting ravines and draws. After soaring along the rim, eagles entered ravines at fast speeds and glided through with wings tucked in fairly close to the body. Although no prey was seen taken in this manner, rock doves (Columba livia) were seen flushing out of the ravines as the eagles flew through. We speculate that any prey, either mammalian or avian, that is exposed and vulnerable may be taken during such quick flights. The adult eagles in Cabin Draw brought from 0 to 4 prey items per day to feed two nestlings. This statement is based on data gathered during observations on four trap days. Avian prey included an adult red-tailed hawk, a fledgling great horned owl (Bubo virginianus) , a nestling ferruginous hawk (Buteo regalis) , a fledgling prairie falcon, a black billed magpie (Pica pica) , a rock dove, and a ring-necked pheasant (Phasianus colchicus) . Mammalian prey brought to the eyrie included one or more Townsend ground squirrels, black-tailed jackrabbits, desert cottontails, and pocket gophers (Thomomys townsendii) . 78 The Black Butte adults hunted from perches on Black Butte, cairns, rock piles, and crossbars of pole lines. Rock piles north of the power lines provided habitat for mammalian prey. A favored area for the flapping-soaring method of hunting was the ravine and bowl west of Black Butte. No data were gathered on food habits at this eyrie. The Feedlot adults hunted from specific power poles located near likely rabbit habitat; the tallest and most dense vegetation in the area. Other hunting perches were on rock piles, Dorsey Butte, and along the canyon rim. During July, the adult female hunted along the slopes and rim southeast of the eyrie using the flapping-soaring method. The male also hunted this area but was not marked with a radio and was more difficult to follow. Both adults hunted just west of the eyrie in the grassy bowl where there were updrafts for soaring. Prey found in the eyrie included remains of black-tailed jackrabbit, Townsend ground squirrel, desert cottontail, white-tailed antelope squirrel (Ammospermophilus leucurus, and an unidentified snake. The relative time the Feedlot adults spent perched versus flying was studied on full day samples (sampling every 10 minutes) between 16 November 1976 and 29 January 1977. During 100 hours on nine sample days, the adult female (with radio) was perched 97 percent, and flying 3 percent of the day-light hours. In 45 hours on eight sample days, the adult male perched 97 percent, and was flying 3 percent of the time. Ground squirrels were not available during this season and rabbits were probably the primary prey. On four days, the male killed one jackrabbit each, and on three occasions, shared it with the female. The small post-breeding range was probably related to two factors: 1) less demand for food, and 2) the location of hunting perches used during the winter months. Daily activities and rhythms. During January and February, eagles were not often seen flying except between perch sites. Most of the flights were flapping flights. The breeding eagles usually roosted at night in the canyon, and were seen flying out to the power lines from dawn to mid-day. On foggy days or windy days with snow, eagles were less likely to leave the canyon. During incubation (March and April) , the females remained at the nest, and males seemed to spend much time near the nest. To minimize human disturbance, we only spot checked at nest sites during these months. After the eggs hatched, the adults at the Ogden, Cabin Draw, and Feedlot eyries tended the young closely until the nestlings were two to three weeks of age. In subsequent weeks, the adults often remained away from the eyrie for intervals of three to six hours from mid-morning until late afternoon. When food was 79 brought to the nest, the adult female spent time at the eyrie, tearing the prey apart and feeding the young. Until the young were about eight weeks old, the female, and sometimes the male, perched within line-of-sight of the eyrie. Night roosts were often in the eyrie ravines where favored perches were easily identified by "whitewash" on the rocks and ground below. The power pole crossbars were commonly used for night roosting, even through the winter. Automobiles passing in the dark beneath the powerline did not flush roosting eagles. Behavior and dispersal of young. The nestlings remained in the eyries into June and July. Young in nest, with no shade and exposure to the afternoon sun, tucked their heads into small crevices or crawled into large crevices in the rocks, apparently to escape the heat. The two young, at the Feedlot eyrie, crawled into a large crevice during the afternoons. As they grew older, the larger bird dominated the shade areas. The younger Feedlot sibling was found on the ground below the eyrie on 24 May after a wind and rain storm, and was also found on the ground on 1 June. On 23 June, it was again found on the ground below the nest, but by this age (10-11 weeks) it was able to glide into the ravine. The older sibling had also recently fledged. The younger bird was placed back in the nest, but it jumped out again the next day. After fledging, the young perched in shaded areas near the eyries during warm afternoons and often used favored perches of the adults. The young remained close to the eyrie and often returned to it to receive food from the adults. Young were very vocal and called throughout the day for food. It appears that the young are provided with food until a certain date. The young then disperse and sometimes wander through the range of other breeders. Carrion, which is easily located by watching the activity of common ravens (Corvus corax) or black- billed magpies, may be an important food source at this stage of development. The young do not use the entire adult home range, but tend to stay closer to the eyrie. The lack of food brought and the short term absence (up to a day) of the adults from near the eyrie, may prompt the young to disperse. The date of dispersal varied between young from different nests and between siblings (Fig. 2). The two young at the Ogden nest were not marked and dispersal date was undetermined. The Black Butte fledgling was found dead on 9 July in a draw by the river. The radio transmitter had been cut off the bird and the bird's head was laterally flattened. The cause of death was not determined but the day before we found the carcass, a local resident reported seeing an eagle with a yellow marker flying along the road in the same vicinity. 80 The two Feedlot young dispersed independently of each other. The younger sibling (band 629-02631), with the radio, was last seen and received on 20 July. A young eagle (unmarked), presumed to be the older sibling (band 629-02630) , was last seen near the eyrie ravine on 30 July. The two young at Cabin Draw were tethered to the nest plat- form from 27 June to 6 July during trapping attempts. The larger young (band 629-02606) fledged on 20 July and was seen in the eyrie ravine during the following week. This eagle was not marked with a radio, and was difficult to find and identify after it left the nest cliff. The smaller sibling (band 629-02605) fledged on 24 July. By 11 August, it was seen perched in the company of the adults on the west end of Jackass Butte (Fig. 4). On subsequent days, the young eagle moved back and forth across the river. On 14 September, this eagle was caught by hand in an emaciated condition (weight 1,400 grams), unable to fly. The bird was fed and cared for until release on 19 October (weight 3,540 g) in apparently healthy condition. This instrumented eagle stayed within the range of the parents through 25 October when it moved west several kilometers. It was chased by two adults at Castle Butte (6 km west of the eyrie on the south side of the river) on 6 November. The bird's radio signal was last detected south in the foothills of the Owyhee Mountains on 17 November, 29 days after its release. Red-tailed Hawk Chronology. Very few red-tailed hawks reside in the Study Area from late September until late February. Nest building and repair began in mid-March and continued in late March. Five pairs of hawks were studied for various intervals and intensities (Fig. 2). Hatching date at the Black Butte nest lagged about two weeks behind hatching at the Halverson (H) and Priest Ranch (P) nests. Our detailed study of the Black Butte (B) nest, as well as less intensive observations of the Spoon (S) and Upper Bigfoot (U) nests, began in June. The adult male RT-1 (color marked and instrumented in May 1975) from the Priest Ranch nest returned to the 1975 home range by 28 February 1976 (J. Messick, pers. comm. ) . RT-1 and mate were seen copulating nine times from 22-24 March and building a new nest 75 m east of the 1975 nest with incubation commencing by 2 April. The adults at the Priest Ranch nest were studied in detail until the nestlings died between 7-12 May. Later information was gathered during spot checks or on prairie falcon sample days. At the Priest nest, two young hatched between 4 and 7 May, but by 13 May, they were dead, possibly from overheating. In an attempt to encourage the Priest Ranch hawks to continue using the site, two young of a brood of four from the Halverson Lake nest were placed in the Priest nest on 15 May. The Priest adults, although remaining in the canyon, did not return to the nest, so the foster young were removed after two days. Even though the nesting attempt failed, the Priest adults remained within their old home range for the duration of a typical breeding season. RT-1 dispersed between 13 and 17 August. RT-1 dispersed on 14 August in 1975. Trapping effort. A total of 101 man-hours and 68 set-hours were expended in May, June, and July to capture one adult red- tailed hawk (Black Butte) . One nestling was instrumented at the Black Butte nest and a second nestling was instrumented at the Upper Bigfoot nest. The 1975 transmitter on RT-1 (Priest Ranch nest) continued to function through the 1976 breeding season. Horizontal space use. Neither adult was marked at the Halverson Lake (H) nest, but in 10 hours of observations during spot checks and trapping visits, a minimum of 4 km^ was observed used (Fig. 6). Adults brought prey from both sides of the Snake River. The eyrie was on a ledge along a vertical cliff and the adults perched on the nest ledge, the rim, and the cliff. The male was darker than the female and we could visually differen- tiate between the sexes without using radios. o The total area used by RT-1 (Priest Ranch nest) was 24 km . Excluding areas used on only one day, the home range was 13 km^ (Fig. 7). There was no change in the home range after the nestlings died. There was a change in time spent out of the canyon. The 1975 home range, while young were present, was 23 km^ and included much of the same area used in 1976. In both years, we monitored from the north side of the canyon, so the exact location of the bird's south side range was not determined, but the bird was seldom noted south of the bluffs above the nest. Favored perch sites for both Priest adults included ledges and rocks near the nest, trees at the Priest Ranch, and rocks and big sage (Artemisia tridentata) along the south slope above the nest. North of the canyon, RT-1 frequently used tall power line poles 226-234 and occasionally perched out to 223-238. In 1975, the extremes were similar, from 225-235. Poles of the short power line were not used for perching during either year. Adults also perched on big sage north of the canyon, as well as on the ground. The canyon cliffs provided updrafts for soaring over the north and south rims. The home range boundaries for the unmarked Spoon and Upper Bigfoot red-tailed hawks were based on a relatively small number of sample hours (Fig. 8) . These home range measurements should be considered as minimum values. 82 ::a:::>'A: Mbr , ^rrr«Tr>-,^» -*> : ::.: ::::x:::: r^;1*: • -1-3- • • • • ?'•• ••'• ■ • 'fa • Gl€**DAb£ . . o . I. - i : (1. . ;1\: w 8 V u ■ ■— -v 1-7 f '* :i6: ?" il V 24' tm\ .7 • ' ^f~;v-^- 21 -1 10 N II \\\ •■ A Nest : : • Farm Fig. 6. Area used (4 km2) by adult red-tailed hawks at the Halverson (H) site during 10 hours of monitoring on 19 April through 22 May, 1976. 83 Monitored 463 hours 18 March-11 August Fig. 7. Area used by RT- 1 , adult male red-tailed hawk, at the Priest Ranch (P) site, 1976. 84 Site Spoon ■— Upper Bigf oo t Area Hours Days Sample period 5 km2 ' 29 5 4-24 June km' 5 5 4-24 June 1 Mi A Nest ::: Farm Perch Fig. 8. Area used by adult red-tailed hawks at the Spoon (S) and Upper Bigfoot (U) sites, 1976. 85 The Spoon (S) adults frequented a home range of 5 km2 during brood rearing. Favored perches near the eyrie were on rocks along and below the rim, within 60 m to the east and west. The adults usually roosted at night on the canyon wall near the eyrie. They avoided flying to the south where an aggressive pair of prairie falcons defended a territory. Crossbars of the small power line between poles 182-198 were used, as were poles 133-137 of the tall power line. These adults soared from the eyrie and hunted south over the desert and farm fields of the flood plain. The Upper Bigfoot (U) adults frequented a home range of 8 km2 during brood rearing (Fig. 8) . Favored perches were along the canyon wall within 25 m of the eyrie and east across the ravine. These adults avoided the area of the rim west of the nest, where a pair of ferruginous hawks nested. The red-tailed hawks also perched on a power line extension that branched south from the short power line at pole 213. They also used poles along the short and tall power lines. The Upper Bigfoot adults were seen soaring over the rims and slopes south and east of the eyrie as well as over the desert and farmed fields toward the river. The radio-tagged male at the Black Butte (B) nest used a home range of 20 km2 during brood rearing, post fledging, and dispersal (Fig. 9). By visual observations, we noted that the unmarked adult female remained within the home range of the male and was often found with the marked male when away from the nest site. The total range of the male was 34 km2 including area for one long soaring flight to the southwest (Fig. 9). The home range of the female (20 km2) was relatively easy to determine because she often flew with the male or with the radio-marked fledgling. The male was reddish in color and the female was regular color. The female had a secondary feather growing on the right wing that was easily recognized until about 9 July. The nest was on a ledge of a 10 m high cliff that was 800 m south of the main canyon wall. Favored perches were on the rim above the eyrie and 100 m west. A row of trees by the river, south of the eyrie, provided shaded day perches for both the adults and the fledgling. Two poles between two farms on the south side of the river were used for early morning hunting, loafing, and night roosting. These poles were in sight of the nest. Both adults perched on hilly ground in areas with sparse vegetation and along a rocky bluff in Sections 28 and 29. Small power line poles and fence posts were also frequently used for perching. From 16 June to 26 August, the location of the Black Butte adult male was noted every 15 minutes on 15 sample days (ranging from 0.75 hour to 10 hour samples on 12 days, to three full day samples). Based on 384 data entries (96 hours of monitoring), the adult male was located on the north side of the river 28 percent, on the south side 69 percent, and close to (within 125 m of) Monitored 138 hours on 27 days from 5 June to 1 September. m y:^- Home range 20 km Total range 34 km Used on only one flight Nest '■'■'•'■ Farm • Perch Area used by adult red-tailed hawks at the Black ) site, 1976 . 00 the eyrie three percent of the time. The location of the adult female sampled on 12 days (when observed) and based on 180 entries (45 hours monitored) was 42 percent on the north side,- 48 percent on the south side of the river and 10 percent near the nest. The adult female was found near the eyrie and/or the young more often than the male and almost always seemed to be within visual distance of the nest during our visits to the nest for trapping or prey collection. From 5 to 16 August, the radio-marked young hawk was on the north side of the river 68 percent, on the south side 32 percent, and near the eyrie 0 percent of the time, based on 100 entries (25 hours monitored) on four days. Hunting behavior. All the red-tailed hawks studied used several methods of hunting and these methods were similar to those used by golden eagles. Red-tailed hawks hunted while watching from perches on cairns, canyon rims, trees and lower vegetation, the ground, and power line crossbars. As with eagles, flights from perches were either direct to the prey or were out over habitat in search of prey, usually to distances less than 100 m. Red-tailed hawks also hunted while flying. Flapping or soaring flights were over greater distances and of longer duration than flights from perches. Soaring flights were common when thermals and winds were favorable (from late morning to late afternoon). Soaring flights lasted for up to an hour and appeared to be used to locate prey. A third method of hunting was a combination of flapping and soaring low along slopes and rocky areas. This method was used by all of the study birds to take lizards. Hunting while hovering was a fourth variation used, usually when hunting in flat terrain over fields of grass or dense vegetation. A fifth method of hunting was a "fly and scramble" technique. Typically, a short flight of less than 15 m followed by a running scramble on the ground was used to capture lizards and snakes. Before and after the nest failed, the adult male at the Priest Ranch site often hunted within the canyon. The trees and fields of the abandoned ranch provided varied habitat for prey. Relatively little time was spent over the desert flats south of the canyon. The adults breeding at the Spoon and Upper Bigfoot sites used similar hunting methods. High soaring flights (20 to 200 m) were made out over the floodplain and the farmland close to the river. Flapping-soaring flights were used along the long slopes 88 of the draws and ravines close to the eyries. Specific sections of the tall and short power lines were used as hunting perches. At the Black Butte site, hunting from the favored perches was common. Hunting while hovering occurred over the desert, grass fields, and along the edge of farm fields. The amount of farmed area (stippled on Fig. 9) within the 20 km home range was 5 km^ . Probably due to the amount of ground cover the hawks did not hunt over the agricultural land until after the crops were harvested (hay fields were mowed periodically) and the height of ground cover was reduced. The "fly and scramble" technique was often used by the Black Butte male during the latter portion of the breeding season (August) when hunting over bluffs in Sections 28 and 29 (Fig. 9). The diversity of prey brought to red-tailed hawk nests was great. For example, prey remains found in the Black Butte nest included: desert cottontail, black-tailed jackrabbit, Townsend ground squirrel, bushy-tailed woodrat (Neotoma cinerea) , deer mouse (Peromyscus maniculatus) , pocket gopher, common flicker (Colaptes spp.), quail (Lophortyx spp.), gopher snake (Pituophis melanoleucus) , western ground snake (Sonora semiannulata) , whip- snake (Masticophis taeniatus) , garter snake (Thamnophis elegans) , bullfrog (Rana catesbeiana) , side-blotched lizard (Uta stansburiana) and western fence lizard (Sceloporus occidentalis) . We often found remains of snakes, small mammals, and rabbits in the other study nests but did not sample and record prey. Daily activities and rhythms. During the incubation period, both the male and female at Priest Ranch were seen separately in incubation posture on the nest. On several occasions, both adults soared together over the north and south canyon rims during the incubation period. The average number of round trips north of the north rim per day by RT-1, as well as the average amount of time spent to the north during the daylight hours is shown in Table 2. After the nestlings died, RT-1 spent less than half as much time to the north and made less than half as many round trips north per day. Throughout the nesting season, RT-1 hunted within the canyon and occasionally flew to the south but we were unable to quantitatively sample those flights. When north of the canyon during the incubation period, RT-1 spent 50 percent of the time flying and 50 percent of the time perched on poles, sage or the ground. The daily rhythm of the portion of the daylight hours spent north of the canyon by RT-1 was also noted (Fig. 10) . During the incubation period, the first flights out of the canyon by RT-1 were usually after 0800. RT-1 was often found out of the canyon from 1700-1800 hours. After the young died, the adult male seldom Table 2. Activities of RT-1, adult male red-tailed hawk, at the Priest Ranch (P) site, 1976. Stage of breeding season Samp le period Hours moni t . Flights NE of canyon moni t . Courtship and nest 18-24 March 55 building Incubation 2-26 April 121 Hatching 5-7 May 40 After death of young Total 15 May-3 August 216 247 14 23 14 51 21 Avg . time spent to NE/day 27% 24% 17% Avg . no trips to NE/day 3.5 3 .9 4.6 1 . 5 Comparab le 1975 data 30 May-17 with young July present 140 47 31% 5 .3 90 0.70 0.60H 0.50 0.40- 0.30 0.20 c o &0.10 a u 0) 1976 Incubation 2-26 April (9 days) Z h \ i — '~l.:'.' i ' 1 r -l 1 r 06 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 s0.401 o £0.30 •o0.20 H c a xO.ioH c p. -•- ; V; I i -*^i £ i 'fit _TSj . . .i. . . ..»-_-._T.-~ :.-»—_-. » 2 I « • • :> ^rrr ;~- t -« —~4'~~-K 7t~ T) . . » N c 3 . .t, • X « * " |j \ j — T- -a 3 * *■- >' ^--y^ ----- iii/ V f .j 7 71 V m A. ^ U- T^T GO H 00 60 C cfl C C -H CO cfl •H 1— I M S-i iH 60 CU ■U T3 O- Q) CO 0) CO £ VH ~'**2id3 ^■•■:::\h: .7. PM T3 O •H M 01 ft ro cn in en -a- r~» VD C^ vD n CTi (3 ^— N 3 -a 1-3 , 60 OJ rH EtJ 1 3 4J cd 1-1 4-1 S CM O . — 1 a 0 1 s-' CN a> c rh 01 0 MH tiO •H 60 C id a 4-1 C -H 01 od cd •H H M M J3 H 60 0) 3 4J TO ft cu 0 CO 01 0) e a 0) iH •H 0 ■H C 4-4 T3 j UTTTTpi>M { - /*-,.-, s h o t:i' y-" it4-"' ^ CO cu 4-1 g CO -H CO S-l Cd M-l •H v£5 C o 0 .-I cfl 14-1 ■H Cfl U ft QJ 3 T3 cfl CO -a cu CO 3 cfl 01 < 00 •H 103 Two mated pairs of falcons from adjacent nests (800 m apart), instrumented in the Eastern Study Site in 1976, used areas similar in size to those of the falcons in the Intensive Study Site (Figures 19-22). From visual observations, the vegetation at the Eastern Study Site was quite different than at the Intensive Study Site. No large grass fields were present within the study birds' home ranges. Generally, the cumulative area used by adult females remained small during incubation then rapidly increased with each sample day beginning a week after hatch. The cumulative area used by adult males increased rapidly during incubation but later slowed as no new areas were used. For example, the Humpback (H) female (Fig. 23) made no flights over 500 m from the nest until 11 May. Even though the cumulative area used by the Humpback female increased with each week of our sampling, to a total of 83 km2, the area flown over on any given day remained between 1-30 km2. In contrast, cumulative area of the Humpback (H) male increased from 1 to 19 May but in June no new areas were used (Fig. 23). A similar behavioral pattern appeared for the Loaf (L) pair but to a lesser degree. Values referred to as "Minimum daily area" were recorded and represent the minimum space used by the bird for that day (Fig. 23). On some days in the Eastern Study Site, we were forced to monitor from a single location because the automobile trails were impassible (muddy) or we concentrated observations on a nearby eagle that remained near the canyon. In the Intensive Study Site, data indicated that an individual bird may cover 20 to 40 km2 on several days but only 1 to 5 km2 on other days (Fig. 24). Composite maps of home ranges of falcons breeding in close proximity illustrate that in both 1975 and 1976, instrumented falcons hunted over some of the same areas. The range overlap and mutual use area for the PF-3 male and the PF-2 female (mated pair) and the PF-1 female (from an adjacent nest) was 48 km2 in 1975 (Fig. 25) . The area of mutual use for three falcons that nested adjacent to each other in 1976 was 47 km . This value is essentially the same as that computed in 1975. The area of mutual use for four falcons in the Intensive Study Site in 1976 was 9 km (Fig. 26) . Probably this area is smaller than computed for three falcons because the scrape of one bird (PF-New) was further west and the hunting flights paralleled those of the other three (Fig. 17). The range overlap of the two adjacent pairs in the Eastern Study Site was 27 km2 (Fig. 27). Although falcons did have overlapping home ranges, not all of the birds were out of the canyon at the same times. Falcons also appear to maintain "individual distance" as they fly within the home range. The term individual distance refers to the area 10 4 \ 4 Km 3 M N 'W^ ^ :X r 4 I 1 J 3 jl ;i i- *3 ! i ';i v '"'' |RW I, ^AH^ ITMf « >:x": : : >: a *T6l_ !Tk^%|M:^B^'*!»^ nestling home range Only 1 flight WWWWWW — \\\\\ Period X^t incubation Period Samp le period Flights Hours Area incubation 29 April-10 May 18/19 48 30" nestling 11 May-13 June 43/49 156 40 fledgling 19-25 June 9/9 45 N.I. home range 70/77 249 35 Fig. 19. Area used by Hd\ adult male prairie falcon, 1976 105 4 Km * A, '' tf i !]ii;i;i;i|iii^iiiiiM!'i-^-,-i-u 'k'i'i1 i i I i i i ■ • Fig. 20 1976. Area used by H$ , adult female prairie falcon 106 ■::T^: Ja& ■* ^i-''" . j> « . i; : : x i*sr„ ;-■ incubation nestling home range vWWWWWW Mir. .... .T^ =*• • iV' •> >i ,. . . . ~p . ... ... B.p sV-l; rr.". . . -. .-: . *■.■ . .. t- , H&frc" ' : ' £ ' " •" " Period Sample period Flights Hours Area :;l;: incubation 29 April-13 May 16/18 76 70 ,/V nestling 19 May-13 June 16/34 129 120 laAoMna 1Q-9S .TnnP 11/11 44 N.I. 43/63 249 78 fledgling 19-25 June home range Fig. 21. Area used by L« n J !!JSjfer:i4?:ii?iii^!i« Fig. 25. Composite of home ranges of 3 adult prairie falcons (PF-10., PF-2£, PF-36*) , 1975 breeding season, Intensive Study Site. 1 11 '|'| i ■ i i i i i U'o.' i iS i ! > ft 1 1,1 i i'i'ii iv • r * • ►..,.....". 'ill! Fig. 26. Composite of home ranges of 4 adult prairie falcons (Co* Cg, No*, PF-22), 1976 breeding season, Intensive Study Site. 112 No . b irds Are us ine 4 Km ft^sijiiiiiSrsi'iiaa^!!!!!! r: : -n: : : : : : l^t-^fV^laHf^" Fig. 27. Composite of home ranges of 4 adult prairie falcons (Ho*, H$ , Lcf, L$ ) , 1976 breeding season, Eastern Study Site. 113 around the falcon which moves with it as the falcon flies over the hunting range. Individual distance is "an area around a bird, which moves with it, has no topographical reference and into which no other individual is allowed to come" (Condor 1949). At present, it appears that the falcons have enough area so that they can hunt without intraspecif ic conflict. Nesting territoriality. From the period prior to egg laying through dispersal of the young, areas around the scrapes were defended by the adult pairs against other adult or young falcons. In the Intensive Study Site, the area defended included favored perches and air space within 50 m of the 100 m tall cliffs. All nest cavities were located on a vertical portion of the canyon wall and the visibility of a perched falcon at an adjacent scrape entrance was limited or prevented by protruding rocks or ledges. During the breeding season, the falcons spent considerable time within the canyon near the nests. The male and female of a pair often soared, flapped, and glided throughout the territory as if to advertise the boundaries and to observe the activities of falcons at adjacent territories which could not be seen well from perches. The spatial arrangement between territories in the Intensive Study Site differed in 1975 and 1976 (Fig. 28). The female from the Camera territory, the male and female from the PF-2 territory, and the female from the PF-1 territory in 1976 were the same falcons that were present at the respective territories in 1975. The 1976 scrape at the PF-2 territory was 35 m further down the cliff than in 1975. The territory did not extend as far to the east in 1976 but did extend further to the west around a vertical ridge in the cliff. Another vertical ridge separated the territories of PF-1 and PF-2 in 1976. The Camera Site falcons occupied a relatively large territory in both years. In 1976, the territories were not fully established until early April. For one week in mid-March, an unmarked pair of falcons perched and courted around a rocky outcrop along the cliffs above the eastern end of the Camera territory but failed to establish a nesting territory. On 3 April, we captured the Camera female for radio-tagging. While we held her away from the scrape and eggs, the Camera male repeatedly defended the scrape, literally shoving and bumping an intruding adult female from the cavity entrance. This intruding female then flew up to the rim and was chased east past Dedication Point by the male from the PF-2 site. An unknown number of non- breeding adults may be available to fill unoccupied nesting sites but it appears that in some areas of the BPNA, there is no more room and the cliffs are saturated. DS (top of cliff) rim DP L' FACE _X b "^v v """ = d 1975 CLIFF (bottom of cliff? talus slope ~— \^ ^l e Nor thwes t _^ Southeast DS (top of cliff) rim DP 19 76 CLIFF FACE (bottom of cliff) talus slope 100 m i i Scrape locations a Dedication Site d b PF-1 e c PF-2 f Camera Dedication Point Unsuccessful pair Fig. 28. Prairie falcon nesting territories in the Intensive Study Site, 1975 and 1976. Vertical territory boundaries are outlined, and scrape locations are indicated by letter. Landmarks are Dedication Site (DS), and Dedication Point (DP). 115 The two breeding pairs studied in the Eastern Study Site nested in cavities on relatively small vertical cliffs (8 to 14 m) . The surrounding terrain was rolling, with some draws and ridges; much different than the sheer cliff habitat in the Intensive Study Site. In the Eastern Study Site, the territories had more horizontal dimension and less vertical dimension (Fig. 29). The horizontal territory was defended against red-tailed hawks and golden eagles as well as other prairie falcons. The nesting density of falcons per kilometer of canyon rim in the Eastern Study Site was much less than in the Intensive Study Site (Study I, 1976 Annual Report). Territorial defense waned during July and adults that remained in the territories after the young dispersed showed a noticeable decrease in vocal responses and aggressive defense against human intrusion. Fledglings that floundered in flight and landed in the territory of another breeding pair were subject to aggressive acts by the adults defending the area. A visitor to the Intensive Study Site reported (Study VII, pers. comm. ) a fledgling falcon passing from one territory to the next. The young intruder was killed in the air by an adult female (presumably PF-2) . It is a moot point as to whether adults can distinguish their own young once the young have fledged. Adults observed during the 1975 and 1976 breeding seasons reacted aggressively to any falcon that was seen entering the territory. We speculate that if we placed a fledgling in an adjacent territory when no adults were around, the young falcon would later be attacked by its parents as the fledgling attempted to return to the natal scrape. Hunting behavior. All falcons studied used several hunting methods. According to our observations and the food habits data from Kochert (1973), the primary prey for falcons in the study area is the Townsend ground squirrel. Our study birds hunted while watching from perches on power line poles, vegetation (sage) and on the ground. As with eagles and hawks, flights from perches were either direct to prey or over favored habitat in search of prey. Low-level searching flights were made out 30 to 80 m from the perches and back. As many as 20 to 30 flights were made a few seconds apart until prey was killed. Falcons commonly hunted from power poles during March and April before vegetation (grasses) grew in exposed areas. At this time, ground squirrels were exposed in openings between patches of sage. The Humpback (H) male and the PF-New (N) male spent a greater portion of time on power poles than the other falcons (Table 4) . Some instrumented falcons were never seen on power poles. As with eagles and hawks, the instrumented falcons that did use the poles had specific sections of the tall or short power lines that were used for perching. 116 N t 500 m (desert flats) £ C_ fjHL ^s^ 1 />s rim t l 1 ^^y^^ / X talus 3 ^"^ slope / (flood plain) Scrape location H Humpback L Loaf Territory size 37 h 24 h Fig. 29. Prairie falcon nesting territories in the Eastern Study Site, 1976. Horizontal territory boundaries are outlined, and scrape locations are indicated by letter 117 Table 4. Average activity of adult prairie falcons per round trip away from the canyon. He? HO_ Ld71 L£ Co* C£ No71 2°. S$* Incubation No . of flights s ampled Avg duration of trip (min.) % t ime in air % t ime on poles % time on ground Avg minutes on ground/ trip Nes t ling No . of flights sampl ed Avg duration of trip (min . ) % time in air % t ime on poles % time on ground Avg minutes on ground/ trip Fledgling No. of flights s ampled Avg duration of trip/min. ) % time in air % time on poles % time on ground Avg minutes on ground/ trip 15 0 16 50 79 69 87 13 1 18 12 9 10 27 42 69 2 3 15 23 56 81 12 64 83 0 18 8 7 0 13 12 7 9 0 13 8 2 47 147 89 82 0 11 5 0 18 26 1 34 -- 49 -- 86 3 -- 11 5 43 18 47 23 46 54 90 0 10 2 14 39 8 82 1 8 11 5 10 63 10 5 23 55 82 5 13 7 85 88 3 2 12 10 5 6 13 4 32 54 81 14 15 35 96 95 2 1 2 4 1 1 Nestling and Fledgling period data have been combined 118 Several methods of hunting while flying through the hunting range were identified. The majority of the flight time was in flapping flight at elevations below 30 m and attacks at ground squirrels were low angle dives. A second method of hunting while airborne was used over relatively small areas (10 to 20 m across) of short vegetation (Bromus tectorum, Poa sandbergii, short Eurotia) among taller vegetation (Artemisia) . Falcons hovered at 5 to 10 m height, dove and flew across the area and hovered again if the attack was unsuccessful. Repeated attempts were made until the prey was captured or the falcon moved on to another area. A third method of hunting while airborne was during soaring flight. It was difficult to say whether or not soaring falcons were hunting or travelling over the area doing something else. Falcons apparently did not hunt within the canyon. By late June, many of the Townsend ground squirrels had gone underground to begin estivation and by 19 July, squirrel sightings were rare. As a result, the primary food source for falcons was gone. Daily activities and rhythms. Individual falcons spent anywhere from zero percent (Humpback female, incubation period) to 43 percent (Loaf female, fledgling period) of the daylight hours out of the canyon at least 100 m from the rim (Fig. 30). We believe most of the time away from the canyon was spent hunting, based on obvious attacks on prey, the large food demand, and the relatively small portion of the daylight hours spent away from the canyon. The averages of some of the activities of instrumented birds during the developmental periods are listed in Table 4. The majority (69 to 96 percent) of the time away from the canyon was spent in the air. The duration of flights varied. PF-New (N) male made one flight out 2 km, made a kill, and returned to the canyon within 11 minutes. In contrast, the Loaf female once stayed out for five hours. Typically, after killing a ground squirrel, a falcon either flew immediately to the canyon or remained on the ground for 5 to 20 minutes, consuming the prey. Sometimes a portion of the prey was consumed near the kill site with the remainder carried to the canyon. Most of the falcons spent an average of 1 to 10 minutes on the ground per trip (Table 4) . Some trips involved several short periods on the ground. The speed of 16 flights by instrumented falcons back to the canyon (presumably with prey) for distances of 8 to 15 km ranged from 23 to 80 km/hr (14 to 50 mph) . Most flights were between 30 to 53 km/hr (18 to 33 mph) . He? 9 42% (3)b Eastern Study Site Lrf1 0.0% (3)b 0.1% (5)a 3 36% (5)a S? 2$ Intensive Study Site C? Co1 No" 5% (9)c 1% (8)c (5 21% (9)c 6(5(53 28% CH)a 13% (ll)a 20% (9)a 38% (9)a 000(5 13% 8% 10% 25% (12)a (12)a (12)a (12)a ©030(530(30 10% (4)b 8% (4)b 43% (2)f 27% 22% (4)b (8)e 39% (3)d 18% (5)b 20% (5)b 12% (7)b Sampling interval (M D T ) a. 0530-2200 d. 0700-2030 b. 0600-2130 e. 0800-1900 c. 0630-2100 f. 0800-2100 333 29% (6)c 37% (2)c 21% (8)e Fig. 30. Percent of the diurnal day spent out of the canyon by adult prairie falcons, 1976. (Maximum number of sample days shown in parenthesis). 120 The average number of round trips made by each bird for each developmental period is listed in Table 5. The male of each pair made more flights per day in each developmental period than the female (except the PF-3 male in 1975) . Although, we cannot say that prey was carried to the canyon on each round trip, we can say that our data represents the maximum possible number of prey carries. The greatest number of trips out of the canyon on a single day was nine by the Humpback male (10 May) when the young were 1.5 weeks old. As the breeding season progressed, the average number of trips by the males decreased. The average number of trips by the female increased from the incubation to the nestling period but then decreased from the nestling to the fledgling period (Table 5). For the Loaf and Humpback pairs, the combined number of trips for each pair increased in the nestling period when compared with the incubation period but then decreased again during the fledgling period. In some cases, the females were making fewer trips out during the fledgling period but spending more time out from the canyon per trip. At this time, it may have been more difficult to capture prey than during the fledgling period. There may also have been reduced opportunity for catching prey because of the growth and maturation of ground cover at this time. Also, the number of available prey within the hunting range was reduced because of mortality and estivation (Study V, pers. comm.). The time and daily rhythm of falcons away from the canyon (nestling period and first half of fledgling period) is shown in Figures 31 to 33. Prairie falcons did not begin hunting as early in the day as eagles or red-tailed hawks. In general, male falcons first flew out from the canyon between 0700-0800 hours (1 to 2 hours after sunrise) and the females began flying out from 0.5 to 1 hours later. Most individuals had a mid-day lull in flights to the north. The probability of finding the falcons out over the desert after the incubation period was greatest in the afternoon. There was some indication of bimodal activity in some individuals but not in others. From an hour before and until sunset, the female falcons returned to the canyon to night roost. During the weeks of sampling, sunrise and sunset times shifted by about 30 minutes. These shifts are indicated on Figures 31 to 33 by the dotted bars at the bottom of each figure along the "time of day" axis near the 0530 and 2200 hours symbols. Night locations for the males of the Humpback, Loaf, and Camera pairs were recorded on 44 occasions prior to the dispersal period. Males occasionally roosted at night away from the canyon. On 14 occasions when the males roosted away from the canyon, the roosts were identified nine times. The Humpback male was found on the same crossbar of the same pole (tall power line) on three nights and on neighboring poles on three other nights. The Loaf male roosted on a small rocky ridge 7 km northeast of 121 Table 5. Average number of round trips north of the canyon per day per bird and per mated pair of prairie falcons, 1976 Study Subject Incubation Nestling Fledgling Ld1 L$ Ho71 H? Cc? C? No71 2$ Average d* Average ? 4.2(5)-r. 0.2(5K4-4 5-5(3)-. 0.0(3)-' 5- 5 0.1(8) 3.7(9) 0.3(9) 4.5 (3 birds, 17 day s ) 0.2 (4 birds , 25 days) Average pair 4 . 7 4.1(9)-, , , 2. 2(9)-r ' A . 9 ( 1 1 )-. 2.6(1 I)-""7 -5 ±5.5 3.9(12) 1 .6(12) 4.3(12) 2.2(12) 4.3 (4 birds 44 days ) 2.2 (4 birds 44 day s ) 6.5 3.7(4)n 1.5(2)-T 5-2 2>7(4)T 3 8 0.9(4)-" 3-8(7)"h-s 4 1.6(7)-T5'4 2.6(7) 2.6(3) 3.2 (4 birds , 20 days) 1.7 (4 birds, 1 4 days ) 4.9 Date cutoffs for each developmental period are the same as listed for each home range figure. The number of days sampled for each period is shown in parenthesis. The samping interval was 0600-2130 hours, MDT . 1975 Comparable data: (13 May-20 June) PF-1? 2.9 PF-2? 3.0 PF-3cP 2.5 3-5.5 122 06 07 08 09 10 II 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 0.50 - 0.40 0.30 - 0.20- 0.10. 0 ud rO, ~ — r J~LT 11 May-20 June - ru "1 1 1 1 1 r 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 r^; — r 06 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 Time of day (00 hours) M.D.T. Fig. 31. Daily rhythm (portion of the diurnal day spent out of the canyon) for adult prairie falcons, Eastern Study Site, 1976. 123 0 60- 0 50 - 0 40 - 0 30 - 0 20 - 0 10 - 0 - c 0(1 SO-. >, a oO 40- <0 30- UH °0 20 - J) 3 °0 10- +J c KUNA a V H'". BONNE ViUE =; _\< I POINT .....-• t \ M MEIBA □ 4 FEATHERVIUE O |k'3j0fy >»„(ri,-,iV 8ENNETT ''«% o i.o, '^fet'ii, MOUNTAIN "-*'-' HOME GRANDV/EW GLENNS FERRY Fig. 34. Dispersal flights by instrumented prairie falcons, 19 76. ^^^■P""^ 10 20 KM 10 15MI GRANDVIEW MAGI Q)V»#S^&_ O SHOSHONfE ho Fig. 35. Dispersal flight of H-12, immature prairie falcon, 1976 128 By monitoring the dispersal flights of six falcons, we have learned that the rate of travel ranged from 18 to 38 km/hr (11 to 24 mph) . The time of day for the initiation of departure flights ranged from 0958 to 1430 hours and the falcons reached the foothills between 1203 and 1639 hours. Four of the six flight paths were within 90 degrees of the prevailing wind direction. We did not see at what elevations our marked birds flew but two non-tagged falcons seen flying into the foothills at Bonneville Point were less than 20 m above the ground. Once the instrumented falcons reached the foothills, they kept on flying further north or east. Our method of intercepting dispersal flights favored flights to the north or east and it is possible that some falcons migrated west or south. We restricted our monitoring from the foothills to approximately 0900 to 1800 hours. Therefore, birds dispersing in early morning or late evening would have been missed. Once the falcons left the canyon, the flights to the foothills were non- stop, lasting two to three hours before the signals were lost. The first of the instrumented adult falcons to leave the study area was the PF-2 female on 28 June. Apparently, the young from this nest had been killed or dispersed and no young were seen in the territory after this date. The Humpback male did not leave his home range until after 10 October when the radio signal was last heard. It is probable that the radio stopped running after 182 days of broadcasting. Two instrumented falcons were found dead. The young falcon (band 987-21955) from the Finger (F) nest was found dead near the nest by Study VII personnel on 17 July. This bird was last detected alive on 14 July. The Loaf (L) female disappeared from the home range between 21 and 23 June. On 10 July, her carcass was found 10 km southeast of the nest (Fig. 34) . The carcass was intact and had no feathers missing, no blood stains, nor noticeable holes. The carcass was x-rayed and no broken bones were seen. Antagonistic Interactions Golden eagle. During 1976, few antagonistic interactions were noted between breeding or non-breeding golden eagles of either sex or age class. Mated pairs were often seen together perched or flying during winter and unmated individuals wandered into and out of the hunting ranges of the breeders. We did not see adult non-mated eagles winter near nests of resident breeding eagles. However, on several occasions, eagles in immature plumage were seen passing near eyries that had been active in 1975 where breeding adults remained. During winter, eagles of various age classes used the same perching sites but not at the same time. 129 During the breeding season, non-breeding adults were seen perched and flying outside the home range of breeding eagles. Apparently there is a reserve of non-breeding adults ("floaters") within the study area. Some support for this statement is that within three days after the disappearance of the adult female from the Black Butte eyrie, two different adult eagles (larger than the male and presumably female) were seen near the eyrie and the male was courting. The courting continued for about a week and an adult female has been seen with the male within the home range since that time. A perched or flying eagle was occasionally harassed by a prairie falcon or common raven but no physical contact was made. By 19 July, many insects (Cimex spp., and Oeciacus spp.) accumulated in the Cabin Draw and Black Butte eyries and parasitized the nestlings. No mortality was noted but the young did scratch and bite, apparently in response to the insect activity. Interactions with humans other than project personnel were not common. Fur trappers walked close to the eyries in winter and spring. A golden eagle toe was found torn off and in a trap at an illegal set within the BPNA. Red-tailed hawk. No antagonistic interactions between red- tailed hawks were noted. The spatial arrangement between nesting pairs seemed to be adequate. Wandering immature red-tailed hawks were allowed to pass through the home ranges of breeding pairs. On several occasions during the dispersal period, "foreign" immatures were seen soaring with our adult study birds. Interactions with other birds or mammals other than prey were minimal. By 10 June, insect parasites accumulated in hawk nests and in the case of the Black Butte Site, prompted the young to jump from the nest prematurely. Interactions with humans were not common. Fur trappers were not in the area during the red-tailed hawk nesting season. Prairie falcon. In the nesting territories, aggressive interactions between adult falcons were common. Any breeding adult that crossed into the nesting territory of another was chased until it left the territorial boundaries. Falcons frequently harassed golden eagles and red-tailed hawks by diving and swooping. Humans that walked the canyon rim did flush breeding adults perched near the rim. If humans were to flush fledglings into an adjacent falcon's territory, the results could be lethal to the young falcon. Adult falcons from the Eastern Study Site often flew within the boundaries of the impact area for a National Guard artillery range but not at times when firing was taking place. 130 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Various forms of assistance to Study III by the following persons is acknowledged: Michael Kochert and Albert Bammann for assistance with contractual and field problems. BLM personnel provided food habits data. Morlan Nelson for assistance in providing lure birds and information pertinent to Study III. John Messick, Lowell Diller, Gayle Sitter, Jon Montan, Eb Evans, Richard Howard, Brad James and Charles Schwartz for field assistance. Robert Montoya for preparing radio backsacks and other field gear when needed. Frank Craighead, Jr. and Bernd Meyburg provided helpful comments about the study while visiting the area. Erik Stauber provided a lure bird for trapping. Morton Awes volunteered pilot time during aerial searches. Clifford Code x-rayed the falcon carcass. LITERATURE CITED Condor, P.J. 1949. Individual distance. Ibis 91:649-655. Dunstan, T.C. and J.F. Harper. 1975. Activity, hunting patterns, territoriality, and social interactions of birds of prey in the Birds of Prey Natural Area. Pages 51-96 in Snake River Birds of Prey Research Proj . Ann. Rep. 1975. U.S. Dept. of Interior, Bureau of Land Management, Boise. 193 pp. Kochert, M.N. 1973. Density, food habits, and reproductive performance of raptors in the Snake River Birds of Prey Natural Area. USDI, Bureau of Land Mgmt. , Boise District Annual Report. 54 pp. 131 Notes : 132 Jon Montan, Study IV-A, conducts a trapping program to determine rodent species composition, density, and distribution in the Study Area (BLM photo by Dunstan) . 133 STUDY IV- A: CONTRACTOR: Density and species composition of small mammals and birds in the Snake River Birds of Prey Natural Area, Idaho. Ecology Center, Utah State University, Logan, Utah 84322 INVESTIGATORS PROJECT SUPPORT: Dr. Michael Wolfe, Principal Investigator. Jon R. Montan, Jr. , Research Associate. Kathleen Montan, Field Aid. Jan Wassink, Field Aid. Martha Wakefield, Field Aid. Brad James, Field Aid. U.S. Department of the Interior, Bureau of Land Management, Contract No. 52500-CT5-1003. OBJECTIVES: 1. Stratify the study area into major vegetation types and land-use patterns from existing aerial photographs and other means. 2. Sub-stratify the major vegetation types into basic land-use types, and sample the major prey species by acceptable statistical and censusing techniques. 3. Specifically, sample for prey species in those areas identified by radio telemetry efforts (Study III) to be intensively hunted by golden eagles, prairie falcons, and red-tailed hawks. Aspects of vegetation composition, cover, physiography, and any other factors will be studied to determine the importance of prey vulnerability to predation. 4. Assess and compare prey densities in the different habitat and land-use types to ascertain the possible effects of different land uses upon prey densities. 5. Ascertain seasonal and yearly changes in prey species' composition and densities. 6. Calculate annual prey population densities in terms of biomass within each strata of the total study area and in areas intensively hunted by raptors. ANNUAL SUMMARY Relative rodent densities were determined at 34 sites within the study area during spring, summer, and fall, 1976. Relative 134 densities were also determined in areas identified by Study III as actively hunted by raptors. Cottontails (Syvilagus nuttali) were trapped at eight sites, including two range-agriculture ecotones. Jackrabbit (Lepus calif ornicus) flushing transects were run in spring and fall with the cooperation of Study I personnel. In addition, 20 transects were walked in June in areas identified by Study III as actively hunted by golden eagles. Marmots (Mar mot a f laviventris) were trapped at Evans Ranch, Melba during spring in an attempt to measure density and biomass at a typical colony. During June, marmot distribution along the canyon wall was recorded by means of a walking survey. Censuses of pheasants (Phasianus colchicus) and passerines were conducted during spring. Chukar partridge (Alectoris graeca) were censused using the recorded rally call method. A vegetation map of the study area was completed and distributed to research personnel. METHODS Vegetation Map The completed vegetation map (1 cm = 0.64 km) was photo- graphically reproduced and distributed to research personnel. Percent ground coverage by major vegetation types and other areas was determined by cutting cut areas of map and weighing them with a Mettler balance. Determination of Prey Densities and Biomass Rodents. Relative rodent densities at 34 sites in the study area were determined by operating kill-trap lines of 25 traps for two nights at each site. The kill-trap index for a particular species and site was calculated as: (captures) (100) (trap nights) - (sprung traps & missing traps) One trap-night was one trap open for one night. Analysis of variance was used to test for differences among habitat types and seasons. If the analysis of variance showed significant differences, then a series of t-tests were used to identify specific habitats with higher numbers of prey. All statistical tests were made at the 0.05 level. Marmots were live-trapped between 23 February and 26 March along a 0.5 km-section of cliff and at a 0.1 ha^ rocky outcrop at Evans Ranch, Melba. Live-trapping provided an estimate of density at a typical colony. In addition, marmot colony distribution was mapped by Martha Wakefield by walking along the base of the canyon wall. 135 Lagomorphs. Jackrabbit flushing transects were walked with the cooperation of Study I personnel during spring and fall. Gates (Gates et al. 1968) transect indices were converted to density estimates (hares/km2) by multiplying by 1.43, the conversion factor applied in northern Utah by Gross et al. (1974). This factor may not apply to this area, thus drive counts would be very desirable to establish the correct factor for this area. Drive counts should be made immediately before spring transects or immediately after fall transects. Cottontail rabbits were live-trapped at eight sites, but not all sites yielded enough captures to permit a Schnabel density estimate. Relative densities were expressed as captures/100 trap- nights. Avian prey species. Pheasants were censused in the major blocks of agriculture within the study area by the crow-count method of Kimball (1949). Spring censuses of passerines were conducted by walking Emlen (1971) transects between sunrise and 0900. Individual transects comprised a quadrat, 1.6 km on a side, with a total linear length of 6.4 km. Chukar partridge distribution was determined during August by floating and/or driving along portions of the Snake River through the study area and playing recorded rally calls at 1.6 km intervals. The number of responses following a call during a 10 minute period can be correlated to flock size (Oeklaus 1976) . RESULTS Vegetation Analysis Percent ground coverage by major vegetation and land-use types is presented in Table 1. Big sagebrush (Artemisia tridentata) associations are grouped in the table because it was often difficult to differentiate between "pure" and "mixed" associations. The map in Figure 1 represents a simplification of the original vegetation map, in which only the major vegetative associations are represented. Rodents Limitations of kill-trap indices. It must be emphasized that a reliable kill-trap index has only been demonstrated for deer mice (Montan 1977). Other rodent species were not captured in sufficient numbers during 1975 to calculate densities. The kill- trap index can only resolve differences among seasons or trapping sites. It bears no relation to kill-trap values for other rodent species. For the purposes of the subsequent discussion, however, kill-trap indices will be assumed valid for other rodent species. 136 Table 1. Estimated percent ground coverage by major vegetative associations of the Snake River Birds of Prey Study Area, 1 November 1976. 2 Description % Coverage km Greasewood-cheatgrass Big sagebrush-cheatgrass Big sagebrush-winterfat 22.3 Big sagebrush-Sandberg bluegrass Farms Shadscale/budsage Snake River and reservoirs Spiny hopsage/shadscale Shadscale-winterfat Mountain Home Air Force Base Cheatgrass Winterfat Cheatgrass-shadscale/greasewood Bruneau Sand Dunes Crested wheatgrass Burn (previously big sagebrush-winterfat) Sandberg bl uegrass Totals 100.0 1937 a Hyphen indicates former is numerically or physically dominant and latter is less frequent or exists as an understory. Slash indicates co-dominance or patches of pure stands of either vegetation type. 30.6 591 8.6 166 7.7 149 6.0 117 18.6 360 18.2 352 2.7 51 2.2 43 1.9 37 0.9 17 0.7 14 0.6 11 0.6 11 0.3 6 0.3 6 0.1 3 0.1 3 Initial Point Murphy 5j) Agriculture 72 Big sagebrush-cheatgrass- Winterfat association Spiny hopsage-shadscale association 0 Shadscale ^ Grease wood Mountain Home Intensive Study Area Birds of Prey Study Area 5 10 5 10 I I I l| I I III ■>«j Fig. 1. Major vegetative associations of the Snake River Birds of Prey Study Area, 1976. 138 Assessment of habitats as contributors of prey. The number of rodent species caught or observed at the 34 sites (Fig. 2) ranged from a high of 12 in the riparian zone along the Snake River to three in an irrigated wheat field (Table 2). The only diurnal rodent species, other than the Townsend ground squirrel (Spermophilus townsendi) , are the white-tailed antelope squirrel (Ammospermophilus leucurus) and the least chipmunk (Eutamius minimus) . Occasionally, kangaroo rats (Dipodomys spp.), meadow voles (Microtus spp.), and woodrats (Neotoma spp.) have been observed during the daytime, but their time spent exposed was brief compared to the Townsend ground squirrel. While Larrison and Johnson (1973) found least chipmunks most abundant in depleted shadscale stands, the results of this study indicate as Fautin (1946) found, that they were restricted to big sagebrush. Chisel-toothed kangaroo rats (Dipodomys microps) were confined to shadscale (Atriplex confertif olia) . Kenagy (1972) showed that this species, because of its tooth morphology, is able to excise hypersaline tissue and feed on the less-saline mesophyll of shadscale leaves. Ord's kangaroo rats (Dipodomys or-lii) were widely distributed but were most common on sandy substratts. Bushy-tailed woodrats (Neotoma cinerea) were found within the canyon proper, on rocky buttes in the Kuna Desert, and in dense greasewood (Sarcobatus vermiculatus) stands along Rabbit Creek near Murphy. Desert woodrats (Neotoma lepida) were found only along the canyon rim and in the dense greasewood of Rabbit Creek. Western harvest mice (Reithrodontomys megalotis) , house mice (Mus musculus) , and meadow voles (Microtus montanus) were only caught on very wet sites, such as riparian habitats or along irrigation ditches. Canyon mice (Peromyscus crinitis) were specific to canyon talus slopes. Deer mice, grasshopper mice (Onychomys leucogaster) , and Great Basin pocket mice (Perognathus parvus) were widely distributed, although deer mice were captured most often. The Snake River represents a zoogeographical barrier to rodents (Davis 1939) . The number of rodent species was lower on the south side of the river except in greasewood habitat. There were no least chipmunks caught or seen on the south side. This species was replaced by the white-tailed antelope squirrel, which did not occur on the north side except near Grand View, where it is presumed to have crossed a bridge. The river has apparently also affected the distribution and speciation of the Townsend ground squirrel. Spermophilus townsendi idahoensis has a continuous distribution north of the river, but is absent on the south side. jS.jt. mollis is found on the south side, but only in a small, local population near Fossil Butte. On the north side, shadscale seemed to support fewer Townsend ground squirrels than other vegetation types. A lack of food grasses and shallow soil was probably responsible. Fig. 2. Distribution of rodent kill-trap sites on the Snake River Birds of Prey study area, 1976. 140 Table 2. Number of rodent species at 34 sites in and around the Snake River Birds of Prey Study Areas, 1976. Species Other Total Site Description caught species seen 1 Burn (North of river) 4 1 5 2 Winterfat (N) 5 5 3 Big safebrush (N) 5 5 4 Cheatgrass (N) 4 1 5 5 Shadscale (N) 2 2 4 6 Greasewood (South of river) 8 8 7 Big sagebrush (S) 3 3 8 Shadscale (S) 3 3 9 Cheatgrass (S) 2 1 3 10 Big sagebrush (Mtn. Home area , N) 4 1 5 11 Shadscale (Sirnco Rd. , N) 2 1 3 12 Spiny hopsage (S) 4 4 13 Big sagebrush-cheatgrass (N) 5 5 14 Big sagebrush-cheatgrass (S) 3 3 15 Big sagebrush-shadscale (N) 2 2 4 16 Shadscale-winterfat (N) 3 3 17 Big sagebrush-winterfat (N) 5 5 18 Big sagebrush-shadscale (S) 4 4 19 Greasewood-cheatgrass (S) 3 3 20 Bi g sagebrush/winterfat-farm (N) 3 3 21 Big sagebrush-farm (Mtn. Home , N) 4 1 5 22 Shadscale-farm (N) 3 3 23 Winterfat- farm (N) 3 3 24 Big sagebrush-farm (S) 4 4 25 Greasewood- farm (S) 3 1 4 26 Siberian wheatgrass (1st spring in 1976, N) 3 1 4 27 Crested wheatgrass-wi ldrye (2 nd spring in 1976, N) 3 1 4 28 Crested v/heatgrass (Old seeding. 10 yrs., N) 3 3 29 Canyon talus (N) 4 1 5 30 Canyon flats (N) 3 1 4 31 Canyon riparian (N) 8 4 12 32 Road effect in big sagebrush (N) 4 1 5 33 Marsh (N) 4 1 5 34 Wheatfield (S) 2 1 3 1-8 = 1975 Live-trapping sites 26- 28 = B.L.M. seedings 9-12= Other range vegetation 29- 31 = Canyon sites 13-19= = Range ecotones 32- 34 = Mi seel 1 aneous 20-25= Range-farm ecotones 141 Other rodent species observed, though not systematically trapped were pocket gophers (Thomomys townsendii) , yellow-bellied marmots, beaver (Castor canadensis), and muskrat (Ondatra zibethica) Pocket gophers occurred in places of adequate soil moisture and with a texture friable enough to allow tunnelling. Irrigated fields, irrigation ditch banks, alluvial soils of the canyon bottom and side streams and certain north-facing slopes in Con Shea Basin and on Sinker Butte harbored these animals. Marmots were found exclusively in close proximity to rocky areas. Talus slopes, boulders on the canyon floor, and lava outcroppings, especially near alfalfa fields supported populations of these sciurids. Beaver and muskrat were restricted to aquatic and riparian habitats along the Snake River. Neither of these species were particularly abundant. an Seasonal density changes. To assess differences among seasons aud sites, the kill-trap values for each species (Tables 3-5) were subjected to an analysis of variance. Kill-trap values for deer mice were converted to densities before the analysis because of the observed disparity between index values for big sagebrush versus other areas (Wolfe and Montan 1976). Deer mice, Ord's kangaroo rats, and chisel toothed kangaroo rats all showed signifi- cant declines as the year progressed. Other rodent species showed no statistically significant seasonal changes. However, one species which obviously became unavailable was the Townsend ground squirrel, due to its July estivation. Effect of native range types and agricultural ecotones. Several species exhibited significant differences among trapping sites (Table 6). Most of these differences have' been previously explained as strong habitat specificity. Deer mice, Ord's kangaroo rats, and Great Basin pocket mice were more eurytypic. For deer mice, big sagebrush and associated ecotones supported significantly higher densities than all other sites. Ecotones between big sagebrush and other range vegetation did not significantly concen- trate deer mice in comparison to pure stands of big sagebrush, nor did big sagebrush-agriculture ecotones when compared to adjacent big sagebrush. Great Basin pocket mice occurred in higher densities at sites within the canyon when compared to all other sites. It is not known why the canyon floor proved so favorable to this species. Ord's kangaroo rats had significantly higher densities along range-agriculture ecotones. It is probable that disturbed soil from roads (around every farm sampled) plus dunes of wind- eroded soil from the fields created favorable burrowing conditions for kangaroo rats (Johnson 1961). Neither the important Townsend ground squirrel nor the potentially important white-tailed antelope squirrel showed significant concentrations in any particular habitat. Capture rates were highly variable, however, and more extensive sampling may have revealed differences. It appears from the data that any concentrations of these species that did occur were strictly sporadic, local phenomena and not typical of a particular habitat. Table 3. Captures/100 trap nights3 at 34 sites 23 March-29 April 1976. in and around the Snake River Birds of Prey Study Area, Site Pm Pp Do St Em 01 Nc Nl Al Dm Rm Mm Mim Pc 1 16.3 1.3 17.5 2 19.2 1.1 2.1 3.1 3 17.6 1.1 2.2 4 14.6 2.4 5 23.3 6 32.7 2.0 4.0 7 30.0 2.5 8 17.5 9 R.9 2.2 10 19.5 11 38.1 12 19.5 9.8 13 6.3 2.1 2.0 11 26.7 15 8.3 16 12.2 2.0 17 29.3 4.9 2.4 4.7 18 2.1 4.3 19 14.9 29.8 20 7.0 25.6 2.3 21 20.5 2.3 2.2 22 33.3 2.0 23 16.2 16.2 14.0 24 20.5 43.6 25 30.2 4.7 26 8.7 2.2 27 16.3 2.3 28 26.2 6.7 29 35.3 11.8 30 23.7 7.9 2.6 31. 31.7 32 23.4 2.1 2.1 33 12.8 34 2.2 2.1 2.5 2.4 2.0 2.0 6.3 5.0 2.4 4.2 4.3 K> 8.8 2.6 2.6 Means 19.5 1.1 5.1 1.2 0.1 0.3 0.1 0.0 0.2 0.5 0.1 0.0 0.3 0.3 See text for definition. See Table 2 for site names. See Appendix for common and specific names. Table 4. Captures/100 trap nightsa at 34 sites in and around the Snake River Birds of Prev Studv Area 28 June-29 July 1976. Siteb Pmc Pp Do St Em 01 Nc Nl Al Dm Rm Mm Mim Pc 16.7 2.3 2.1 4.4 2.1 2.1 2.8 11.9 2.2 6.5 2.9 3.0 2.3 9.1 2.0 8.3 2.0 2.9 16.7 2.9 2.2 6.1 1 27.1 4.2 2 18.2 4.6 3 6.5 8.7 4 2.1 4.3 5 4.2 6 7 9 9 10 29.6 4.6 11 6.1 12 13 8.6 14 15 8.5 16 2.1 17 16.0 18 19 20 2.2 21 23.9 22 10.9 23 6.8 24 2.9 25 26.8 26 8.5 27 2.2 28 8.2 29 26.2 4.8 30 4.4 31 28.6 32 12.5 33 34 10.3 2.0 2.1 13.0 13.9 3.2 2.2 2.2 2.3 5.7 16.7 2.2 8.3 2.4 4.1 2.4 H.9 2.4 2.4 Means 8.7 1.1 2.4 0.6 0.5 0.5 0.1 0.1 1.4 0.2 0.1 0.1 0.0 0.4 See text for definition. See Table 2 for site names. cSee Appendix for common and specific names. Table 5. Captures/100 trap nights3 at 34 sites in and around the Snake River Birds of Prey Study Area, 7 September-30 September 1976. Site Prri Pp Do St Em 01 Nc Nl Al Dm Rm Mm Mi'm Pc 1 2 4.0 3 8.5 4 5 8.0 6 17.8 7 2.2 8 9 2.0 10 22.9 11 4.2 12 13 14 15 4.2 16 2.0 17 18 19 6.3 20 4.4 21 20.4 22 8.2 2 3 6.5 24 6.4 25 38.7 25 2.0 27 28 8.0 29 4.4 30 5.3 31 7.7 32 7.1 33 34 20.0 2.2 6.3 4.0 2.1 2.1 2.0 2.2 4.8 6.7 4.4 2.2 2.1 4.2 2.0 2.1 2.0 2.0 2.4 4.3 4.2 4.1 4.3 2.0 2.6 7.7 4.2 2.2 -P- -P- 2.2 Means 6.5 0.7 0.5 0,0 0.1 0.3 0.0 0.0 0.6 0.0 0.1 0.4 0.1 0.1 See text for definition. See Table 2 for site names. 'See Appendix for common and specific names. 145 Table 6. Analysis of variance F-values for rodent ki 11 -trapping data from 34 sites in and around the Snake River Birds of Prey Study Area, 1976. Species Season Site Peromyscus maniculatus Dipodomys ordii Perognathus parvus Spermophilus townsendii Eutanii us minimus Onyrhomys leucogaster leotoma cinerea leotoma lepida Ammospermophi 1 us leucurus Dipodomys mi crops Reithrodontomys megalotis Mus musculus Microtus montanus Peromyscus crinitis 14. 8a 3.9b 6.la 1.9b 0.5 1.9b 2.4 1.1 2.0 2.7b 0.9 1.6 1.2 0.9 1.0 1.0 2.5 1.4 3.6a 2.5b 0.0 2.3b 1.5 1.0 0.5 1.0 1.0 7.1b aValues larger than 3.15 (p = 0.05; df = 2,66) indicate a signifi cant difference among seasons. Values larger than 1.6 (p = 0.05; df = 33,66) indicate a signifi cant difference among trapping sites." 146 Effect of livestock grazing. Due to the lack of non-grazed control sites, the experimental design did not include an evaluation of grazing effects. Work by Larrison and Johnson (1973), Phillips (1936) , and Quast (1948) have shown that range depletion tends to diminish the numbers of western harvest mice and Great Basin pocket mice, and increase the number of deer mice. Grazing in wet sites limits the distribution of meadow voles by eliminating the thick grass cover under which runways are constructed. Linsdale (1946) and Howard (1953) found that heavy grazing favors increased numbers of ground squirrels in California. Johnson and Melquist (1976) have noted similar effects in the Birds of Prey study area. Miscellaneous effects. Several special effects could not be substantiated statistically, but nevertheless showed results worth noting. Roads constructed in big sagebrush created suitable burrowing conditions for Ord's kangaroo rats (Table 4), providing travel lanes across unfavorable habitats. This observation has also been made by Johnson (1961) . Range-fire burns seemed to attract higher numbers of deer mice during the summers of 1975 and 1976 than adjacent big sagebrush and winterfat (Eurotia lanata) stands. BLM range seedings following range fires (sites 26-28) seemed to support more deer mice and Townsend ground squirrels as the seedings aged. As summer progressed, these differences became less pronounced. A planted wheat field south of the river did not harbor any rodents until the wheat grew high enough to provide cover. The ubiquitous deer mice then moved into the interior of the field. Later in summer, meadow voles colonized the field under large-diameter irrigation feeder pipes. By fall, deer mice had established a large breeding population. When the wheat was cut, the rodents' vulnerability increased, and raptors, particularly red-tailed hawks, were seen hunting the field extensively. Rodent abundance in intensively hunted areas. During June, Martha Wakefield ran eight rodent trap lines in areas Identified by Study III as intensively hunted by raptors (Table 7). More Townsend ground squirrels were caught in these areas than at 23 sites in different habitats north of the river. Grasshopper mice were also caught more often in raptor hunting areas, however, these mice seldom appear in raptor diets. Marmots. Yellow-bellied marmots occurred exclusively on or near rocky areas. Talus slopes, boulders on the canyon floor, and lava outcroppings, especially near alfalfa fields, supported colonies of these sciurids. Colony distribution was mapped by walking along the base of the canyon wall. The route surveyed extended from Evans Ranch to a point 3.2 km upstream of Swan Falls Dam on the north side of the river, and from Swan Falls Dam to the Priest Ranch on the south side. The only places marmots were seen were near Evans Ranch, along the base of Jensen Cliff, near Swan Falls Village, and in the old fields of Priest Ranch. All of these sites are adjacent to irrigated fields or other sources of succulent vegetation. 147 Table 7. Relative rodent densities (captures/100 trap-nights) in raptor hunting areas. 1976. Snake River Birds of Prey Studv Area. Dates Site Grid Location Pm St 01 Em Do June 3-4 Prairie falcon hunting area (Sand Creek) 5-6 Near Swan Falls R 10-11 Trio Butte area 10-11 Trio Butte area 12-13 Coyote Butte area 15-16 Chattin Flats 15-16 Little Baja 15-16 Dorsey Butte (37.5,96.0) 23.3 6.5 (31,3,92.0) 8.9 4.3 (31.55,92.4) 11.9 2.3 2.4 (32.5,92.5) 10.6 2.1 (32.3,93.5) (46.5,75.0) (46.6,75.8) (48.2,77.25) 7.3 4.7 2.4 2.4 4.3 8.5 8.5 8.5 4.2 2.1 2.1 Means Spring, 1976 means for rodent transects north of Snake River 9.9 2.5 1.9 0.3 1.3 20.0 1.8 0.5 0.3a 3.2 Seven big sage transects with ecotones (only habitat in which Eutamius minimus is found). 148 A colony assumed to be typical was live-trapped between 23 February and 26 March at Evans Ranch, Melba. In a 2.5 ha area a Schnabel population estimate showed a population of 40 marmots, or about 15.9 animals/ha. The sex ratio of the catch approximated unity (14 males and 13 females) . Trapping was purposely terminated before the juveniles emerged from their natal burrows to avoid biasing capture data with a preponderance of trap-pone juveniles. Mr. Evans reported that a particular marmot was seen repeatedly throughout the winter at the ranch, however, the first observation by field personnel was on 29 January. If one arbitrarily chooses 1 February as the adult emergence date, then 1 April was the approximate date of juvenile emergence from natal burrows, according to the breeding schedule determined by Nee (1969) . Marmots were last seen in early August, although most began estivation during July. Voles (Microtus montanus) . Meadow voles were removal trapped for five days in a 53 m^ area along an irrigation ditch at Evans Ranch, Melba. Captures leveled off at four animals on the third day but rose to eight voles on the fifth day, perhaps a result of new individuals occupying areas left by trapped voles. In any case, the vole density is potentially great; on the order of 750- 1500 voles/ha. Using an average weight of 29.3 g, this yields an estimate of 21.9-43.9 kg/ha. It should be emphasized that these figures refer to a small area of favorable habitat, and not to a large, continuous vegetation type. Voles seem to require a growth of grass high enough to lodge and form a mat under which runways are constructed. Areas heavily cropped by cattle near the ditch showed little or no vole activity. The presence of dead Russian thistle (Salsola kali) in the ditch also provided cover for voles. Lagomorphs Cottontails. Table 8 summarizes cottontail live-trapping efforts. At some sites, the number of animals caught was insuffi- cient to permit a density estimate, therefore, captures/100 trapnights have been included to show relative densities. Woodrats (Neotoma spp.), marmots, and jackrabbits were also caught (Table 9) . Rarely were there enough captures of these species to calculate densities. Of particular significance is the fact that the greasewood vegetation type can support significant numbers of cottontails. Big sagebrush does not harbor many cottontails. The few that were caught in big sagebrush were near small rocky outcrops. Jackrabbits. Flushing transects walked in the spring and fall produced ambiguous results (Table 10). In theory, the index should have been lower in spring 1976 than fall 1975, and higher in fall 1976 than spring 1976. That it was not may indicate a flaw in the method itself. One problem is that not all of the 61 transects are in good jackrabbit habitat. Therefore, during years Table 8. Lagomorph live-trapping summary. Snake River Birds of Prey study area, 1976. Grid location Description S.n. L.c. c ■ 1 Spnr. j Capt./lOO t-n S.n. L.c. Density (animals (S.n. or /ha) iy) S.n. L.c. 2 Summer Capt./lOO t-n S.n. L.c. Den s i ty S.n. L.c. lm - 3 Winter Capt./lOO t-n S.n. L.c. Density (32.5,92.4) Trio Butte 5 0 3.3 0.0 5 0 7.1 0.0 0.5 1 3 1.2 3.6 (32.1,87.1) Canyon talus 15 0 10.0 0.0 1.9 11 0 11.0 0.0 1.1 8 0 5.2 0 2.0 (30.3,90.8) Canyon riparian 16 0 10.7 0.0 2.3 9 0 9.0 0.0 1.1 13 0 10.5 0 3.6 (32.3,88.5) Canyon rim 3 0 1.9 0.0 8 1 8.0 1.0 0.8 2 0 1.9 0 ^3 (29.3,93.6) Big sagebrush 1 1 0.6 0.6 1 1 1.4 1.4 0 0 0 0 (29.1,93.1) Big sagebrush- farm ecotone 1 . 1 0.6 0.6 2 6 2.9 8.6 3 1 3.2 1.1 (20.4,95.4) Greasewood- farm ecotone 9 1 4.5 0.5 7 0 4.7 0.0 1.8 2 0 2.1 0 (23.2,86.9) Greasewood 4 3 2.0 1.5 0 2 0.0 1.3 0 0 0 0 Spring: 8 May-26 June Summer: 4 August-10 September Winter: 2-31 December Table 9. Marmot, woodrat, and skunk capture summary. Snake River Birds of Prey study area, 1976. — ■ Grid location Description M.f. N.c Spri N.l 1 ig . S.p. M.m. M.f. N.c Summer N.l. S.p. M.m. M.f. Win N.c. ter3 N.l. S.p.' (32.5,92.4) Trio Butte 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 0 1 0 0 3 0 0 (32.1,87.1) Canyon talus 3 3 0 0 0 0 4 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 (30.3,90.8) Canyon riparian 3 1 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 (32.3,88.5) Canyon rim n 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 0 0 (29.3,93.6) Big sagebrush 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 (29.1,93.1) Big sagebrush- farm ecotone 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 (20.4,95.4) Greasewood- farm ecotone 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 0 0 0 0 (23.2,86.9) Greasewood 0 1 2 0 0 0 0 2 0 0 0 0 0 0 Spring; 8 May-26 June ? Summer: 4 August-10 September 3 Winter: 2-31 December o Table 10. Jackrabbit population indices study area, 1972-1976. Snake River Birds of Prey Natural Area Year Season No. transects No. jackrabbits observed (n) index (N) Jackrabbits/ km2 1972 Fall 1973 Fall 1974 Spring 1974 Fall 1975 Spring 1975 Fall 1976 Spring 1976 Fall 26 36 34 59 60 60 61 61 9 6 4 12 7 11 21 18 43.8 19.0 12.3 31.2 12.5 19.4 57.7 34. 0a 62.6 27.2 17.6 44.6 17.9 27.7 82.5 48. 6a Includes 2 transects where only 3 animals were seen. 152 of low jackrabbit numbers the effective sampling ability of the transects is so low that the transects fail to document population changes. Martha Wakefield walked 20 transects during June in areas identified by Study III as intensively hunted by golden eagles (Table 11) . Jackrabbit densities were significantly lower in these areas than the average density for the study area. The t-value obtained when the mean index value (4.5) for the intensively hunted areas was compared to the spring index value (34.0) for the entire study area was 4.46. Perhaps golden eagles do not respond to hare density but rather hare vulnerability. Areas (such as short grass or winterfat) where hares are most vulnerable are also areas where the fewest hares will be found. In contrast, big sagebrush and greasewood are good hare habitats but vulnerability to avian predators is relatively low. The management significance for golden eagles is that perhaps, as a mitigating practice, the BLM should seek to increase the amount of ecotone between big sagebrush (or greasewood) and grass. Further prey vulnerability studies should shed more light on this question. Avian Prey Species Pheasants. Pheasant crow count routes were driven by Jan Wassink between 1 and 14 May. Two measures of pheasant abundance are provided: no. calls/station and no. birds seen/linear kilometer driven (Table 12). Kimball (1949) described a method of calculating densities from crow count indices based on the distance a crowing cock can be heard. The problem with this method in the study area is that topography and vegetative cover differ too greatly to assume that crowing cocks can be heard within the same radius at each station. Pheasants showed relative densities similar to those observed during the previous field season. The mean number of birds per kilometer observed on all routes in the spring of 1975 (0.34) was not significantly different (t = 0.82; df = 13; p k yrs old) k6 971 . 5 U2.T 21 900. k 39-0 Body weights(g; Males Females n X SD n X SD newborn* 30 19.23 3.2li 22 19.19 3.1+8 young (l yr old) 1 50 — <> 69.6 33.1* juveniles (2 yrs old) 12 235.5 67.8 10 211.2 70.7 subadults (3 yrs old) 21 1+03.8 9I+.2 23 371.3 130.7 adults (>h yrs old) k6 667.1 109. T 21 577-5 123.1+ *born in captivity Ik 12 10 8 6 h 1+6 50 51* 53 Young 10 It 2 173 MALE 62 66 TO T'i Juvenile 78 82 t Subadult _! I 1 I 90 9^ 96 102 106 Adult Snout-Vent Length in Centimeters FEMALE 50 5k 58 Young 62 66 TO Juvenile 7h T8 82 Subadult 86 90 9^ Adult Snout-Vent Length in Centimeters 102 Fig. 1. Western Rattlesnake size class histograms. 174 This would tend to indicate that the greatest difference in growth rate between males and females occurs from the third to fourth year. Growth after reaching maturity is minimal (Fitch, 1949; Fitch and Glading, 1947). As indicated in Figure 1, female rattlesnakes reach their mature size in four years. However, two of the nine captive gravid females were three year olds as indicated by their snout- vent lengths. This would indicate either that some females reach sexual maturity in three years or that some mature females are stunted in growth. Nine captive females produced a mean clutch of 7.5 (range 4 to 10) with about 90 percent of the newborns surviving long enough to be released on the study area. This would indicate that the western rattlesnake in the BPNA has a low reproductive rate, because along with few young per female, they also only bred every other year (Klauber, 1972). Size classes and age structure for gopher snakes are indicated in Table 5 and Figure 2. Although it appears there is a slight difference in size between males and females, with the males again being larger, the difference is not great enough to separate the two sexes in a size histogram. Once again four size classes can be identified. The size class histogram for gopher snakes follows a logical pattern with the exception that subadults outnumber both young and juveniles. Again this difference can probably be explained in terms of activity patterns or detectibility. It is of interest to note that there were fewer adults found than sub- adults, which might indicate that adult gopher snakes have a higher mortality rate than that indicated for adult rattlesnakes. Gopher snakes demonstrated a balanced sex ratio for each age class with the exception of adult size class where males outnumber females 3:1. This difference may be due to unequal mortality rates, or merely differences in detectibility or activity patterns. Growth rates as indicated by the mean body weights of the different age classes (Table 5) gives a mean increase of 68, 121, and 129 grams between each successive age class. The corresponding values calculated for rattlesnakes are greater, although the percentage increase in rattlesnakes is less. Few reproductive data on gopher snakes are available as yet. Snake Food Habits Food habits for gopher and rattlesnakes are indicated in Table 6. All the field collected scats were included under rattle- snakes because they were collected in areas of high rattlesnake density and their size and shape would indicate they were deposited by rattlesnakes. A few of these scats may have been deposited by large gopher snakes. It is of interest to note that 51 out of Table S. Gopher snake size classes and age structure young (l yr old) juveniles (2 yrs old) subadults (3 yrs old) adults {>h yrs old) Snout-vent lengths (mm) Body Weight (grams ) n x SD n x SD 13 386.5 39-9 13 18. T 5-7 12 676.7 72. U 11 86.5 38.9 37 89'k6 82.3 35 207.8 78.7 27 111^.7 5^.2 25 U36.U 76.O Ik 12 J _| l_ 30 35 k0 1*5 50 1 1 1 !_ 55 60 65 TO 75 i i i i i 30 85 90 95 100 _j i i i i 105 110 115 120 125 130 Snout-Vent Length in Cent ir-eters . -4 Fig. 2. Gopher snake size class histogram. 177 Table 6. Snake food habits Prey Item Townsend ground squirrel Nuttall ' s cotton- tail rabbit Deer mouse Western Harvest Mouse Great Basin Pocket Mouse House mouse Antelope Ground Squirrel Muskrat Whiptail lizard Side-blotched lizard ' Western Rattlesnake Scat Palp. Anal. Total Gopher Snake 13 f38 51 Scat Palp.. Anal. Total 3 1 6 1+ Grand Total '■'. 7 1+ 2 1 1 1 Great Basin spadefoot toad Unidentified bird egg *includes 2h field collected scats 178 59 prey items identified for rattlesnakes were Townsend ground squirrels. Also, with only one exception, adult rattlesnakes exclusively took Townsend ground squirrels. In comparison, gopher snakes took a variety of prey items with no clear prey selection evident. The size of the ground squirrels (palpated from snakes) ranged from 70 to 200 grams, while the cottontail rabbits ranged from 115 to 190 grams. Snakes were found to ingest prey as large as their own body weight, but most prey items were about half their own body weight. Snake Growth Efficiency and Prey Consumption The growth efficiencies given in Table 7 are calculated in terms of increase in snake weight in relation to whole weight of the prey consumed. Although the values are still preliminary, it is possible to calculate the estimated prey consumption for each age class of gopher and rattlesnakes that was discussed earlier (Table 4 and 5). Thus between their second and third year rattle- snakes would consume a mean of 840 grams of prey. Between their third and fourth year, male rattlesnakes would consume 1,315 grams, while females would consume 1,030 grams. Gopher snakes would consume a mean of 400, 712, and 759 grams of prey between their second, third, and fourth years respectively. Although they are preliminary these data compare favorably with the scanty data in the literature where estimates of annual consumption rates range from two times the snake's weight (Fitch, 1948) to 4.5 times the snake's weight (Klauber, 1972). LITERATURE CITED Fitch, H.S. 1948. Ecology of the California ground squirrel on grazing lands. Am. Midi. Nat. 39(3) :513-596. . 1949. Study of snake populations in Central California. Am. Midi. Nat. 41(3) :513-579. Fitch, H.S., and B. Glading. 1947. A field study of a rattlesnake population. Calif. Fish and Game. 33(2) :103-123. Klauber, L.M. 1972. Rattlesnakes: their habits, life history and influence on mankind. 2nd' Ed. Univ. of Calif. Press. 2 Volumes. 179 Table 7. Consumption rates and growth efficiencies of captive snakes. Species Feeding Period (days) Body Start Weight (g) Finish Prey Consumed (g) f wt(g)/ day 'on sump, g/day Growth Efficiency Gopher snake 51 52 71 ^3 0.13 0.8U .21 97 122 175 20U 0.55 2.10 .26 kk 21U 253 107 0.39 2.U3 .36 106 1U5 168 225 0.22 2.12 .10 65 15I* 181* 260 0.^6 U . 00 .12 100 190 199 211* 0.09 2.1k .0k 137 319 3U6 205 0.19 1.50 Me .13 an .17 Western Rattlesnake 117 105 151+ 21k 220 216 273 9h O.56 0.02 2.33 0.90 .23 .02 9 ; 170 172 85 0.02 0.91 .02 JO 100 126 85 0.87 2.83 ■ 31 32 208 232 59 0.75 1.31+ .1*1 Me an .20 180 -W-; -33**l|j(P jyiMi: Mwi 'Jj^lpilpw*1^' ■«m John Messick, Study IV-B, records movements and activity patterns of one of the radio-collared badgers on the Study Area. (BLM photo by Dunstan) 181 STUDY IV-B: Dynamics of predation upon a raptor prey base in the Snake River Birds of Prey Natural Area, Idaho. CONTRACTOR: INVESTIGATORS : PROJECT SUPPORT: Idaho Cooperative Wildlife Research Unit, University of Idaho, Moscow, Idaho 83843. Maurice G. Hornocker, Principal Investigator. John P. Messick, Research Associate. Jackson S. Whitman, Randall M. Olson, and Graham W. Smith, Research Aids. U.S. Department of the Interior, Bureau of Land Management, Contract No. 52500-CT5-1004. OBJECTIVES: 2. Attempt to ascertain the density, sex and age structure, and other population parameters of badgers (Taxidea taxus) on the Snake River Birds of Prey Natural Area, Idaho. Explore the strategy of habitat use and describe the social organization of the badger population. 3. Gather information on the food habits of badgers as an aid to estimating the impact of this predator. 4. Collect information on the food habits of other important carnivores, coyotes (Canis latrans) and bobcats (Lynx rufus) . ANNUAL SUMMARY By the end of 1976, the second year of this project, 8,023 trap days had produced 234 captures and recaptures representing 154 different badgers. Eighty-one were males and 73 were females. Fifty-two were identified as young-of-the-year , while 22 of 47 adult females were pregnant, lactating, or showed evidence of recent pregnancy. Of five females captured in both 1975 and 1976, two were probably not productive either year, three were productive in only one of the years, and one animal appeared to have raised young during both 1975 and 1976. Part of all of seven different family groups were captured and recaptured. Captures were made in an area of 35 km2, but were concentrated in 13 km2. This suggests a density of over four per km2. Young moved as far as 30 km from their natal area while adults confined movements to areas usually less than 1 km2. Radio-tracking of 27 different badgers for periods of up to 22 weeks revealed that badgers often returned to previously used dens and decreased their activity in November and December. Townsend ground squirrels (Spermophilus townsendi) occurred in 191 of 231 scats and were more important than smaller 132 rodents, reptiles, and various insects. Analysis of 46 coyote scats revealed jackrabbits (Lepus calif ornicus) and Townsend ground squirrels to be the most important foods for this species. Carcass collections for reproductive, age, and food habits data now total 115 badgers, 73 coyotes, and five bobcats. We have collected 289 badger, 173 coyote, and five bobcat scats. Field work will continue for nine additional months. RESULTS This report marks the end of the second year of field work on this project. Our effort has been directed toward the American badger because of the animal's importance as a predator and apparently high population density. The badger is the major competitor with the prairie falcon (Falco mexicanus) in predation on Townsend ground squirrels. Furthermore, research on the American badger is meager compared to its European counterpart and other carnivores of similar size and distribution. Work on coyotes and bobcats has largely been limited to qualitative food habits, although we will increase attention to coyotes during 1977, utilizing extensive methods and survey techniques to estimate population trends. Our methods were reported in the Annual Report for 1975 (Messick et al. 1975). Much data remains to be analyzed, so this report includes only part of the information which will be available later. Field work will continue through September 1977. Badger Segment Population structure and density. In Tables 1 and 2, I summarize the results of our capture and marking efforts for the past year and also for 1975. Thus far, 8,023 trap days have produced 234 captures and recaptures representing 154 different badgers. Trapping was supplemented by spotlighting, daytime netting, and various other methods. We have frequently used more than one trap at a set, so the ratio of trap days to captures actually underestimates trapping success, especially during 1976 (Table 2). Essentially all animals marked and released in this project have been taken in an area of less than 35 km2 (Fig. 1), but approximately 80 percent of the capture effort and captures have occurred in a much smaller area of 13 km2 . One hundred fifty-four 2 9 captures xn 35 km suggests four marked animals in each kmz . If we use 123 captures (i.e., 80 percent of 154) in 13 km2, the density is over nine per km2. Such elementary arithmetic ignores births, deaths, emigration and immigration, but does give a rough estimate of badger numbers in the intensive study area. The population appears to be stable, so the annual population increase Year Table 1. Numbers and classification of badgers captured by various methods during 1975 and 1976, on and near the Snake River Birds of Prey Natural Area, Idaho. Adults Young-of- the-year luwlb $f's 99' s (5&*'s 991 s Adults YOY d8% 991 s I CD 1/1 4- c/> > QJ CD i— to - CD =5 CD 3 S8t -M CX 3 Q_ +J CO -M CD o Q_ u CL O tO CD rO O) CJ d; C_) S- 1975 35 28 6 6 63 12 41 34 75 24 99 19?6 20 19 20 20 39 40 40 39 14 79 56 135 Total 55 47 26 26 102 52 81 73 22 154 80 234 OO 184 Table 2. Method of capture for badgers during 1975 and 1976 on and near the Snake River Birds of Prey Natural Area, Idaho. Other Year Trap Trap days Spotlight K]?! (flooding, netting dens, etc.) 1975 68 2,793 18 11 1976 106 5.230 15 Total 174 8,023 27 26 NOTE: One hundred fifty-four badgers captured 234 times. 135 G Melba N A Fig. 1. Intensive study area for badger population studies, on and adjacent to the Snake River Birds of Prey Natural Area, Idaho. 136 created by young is probably balanced by higher mortality in this age class. Similar calculations considering only the adult, resident segment of 102 marked animals will yield values of two and six badgers per km^ for the larger and smaller areas, respectively. Few estimates of badger densities exist, but Lindzey (1971) thought that Curlew Valley, Utah and Idaho, might support in excess of one badger per mi (one per 0.4 km ) . The intensive study site (Fig. 1) has more badgers per unit area than nearly any other area on, or adjacent to, the Snake River Birds of Prey Natural Area. Indeed, our observations suggest quite low badger densities on the south side of the Snake River and in certain other areas. We plan to use counts of badger digs as an index to relative numbers on these sites. The population is composed of similar numbers of males and females (Table 1). This is consistent with Crowe's data (Crowe 1975). He obtained carcasses from hunters, trappers, and fur buyers in Wyoming. Lindzey (1971) had 22 males and 29 females in his carcass collection. Eighteen, or 35 percent, were young-of-the-year . Our results show that young-of-the-year contribute 34 percent to our marked population. After tooth sections are completed and inter- preted, the age structure given in Table 1 should be more detailed and perhaps the ratio of young to adults will change slightly. Reproduction and dispersal of young. One-half of the mature females were judged productive, that is, they were pregnant, lactating, or showed evidence of recent pregnancy (Table 1). Of five females captured in both 1975 and 1976, two were probably not productive either year, three were productive in only one of the years, and one animal appeared to have raised young during both 1975 and 1976. During 1976, part or all of seven different family groups (female with young) were captured and recaptured. Females No. 19 and No. 98 (both with young) were radio-tracked during the maternal period. Additional females (Nos. 21 and 76) were radio-tracked during the spring of 1975 and 1976, but we did not capture their young if they raised families (Table 3) . Litter size was usually two or three, and births occurred in March and April. We are uncertain of exact parturition dates. The families we monitored utilized a series of maternal dens. For example, No. 19 utilized only one den from 14 March through 8 April 1976. She emerged, presumably to hunt, approximately every other day, although she frequently was away from this den. She never spent more than two days underground without emerging. Most above-ground activity occurred at night. Later in April and May, No. 19 and her young utilized other dens for spans of four to seven days. '"» ■ ■ >'''-'—**mmmmmmim^i~i^mmmmmmmmmmmmmmmmmmmmmmmmmmmmmmmmmmmmmmmmmmm Table 3. Summary of badgers radiotracked during 1975 and 1976 on and near the Snake River Birds of Prey Natural Area, Idaho. 0) 41 d u 4-> r. -a cq o> a> ni «Q -h nj cu 3 t3 cq jj ai a) roa) co a> • h 4J u DVjO>njoa>oo i C a) t3 TdTStfl C-h CMdCxla) i-i m-i a) -Ma) cocdoj en w -u M cu-uo oo S ts i-i £*, M *J (B S CBSOgM-ixlE-HC w I 3 -ao njocdg a) a) 3 u m 3 o 3S M M 4-) 13-Mgo xi T3B0J E ctj B M M i— I Pi CU 4J 4-1 -H -rH -H -C -r-l CO CD -H CD -H CO M Cd X3d ij co me: . a 4J cm ■ • x c :>, x >, m x >^ cu e OCU-OO « S a3 O OOcom-iS o OcrJOcrJcdcdOcdcdB CD 3M-i q-h hJB SBWOAi Z SSO'd^xlM-i'i'dcu oi 5 M A 09 Mar 75 04 Jun 75 70 1.0 25 12 12 12 — Radio recovered 8 F A IS Mar 75 05 Jun 75 74 0.3 24 12 16 23 — Radio recovered both 23 Jun 76 16 Jul 76 18 0.6 11 2 2 2 1 years. With young in 75? Used same area both years. 6 — 5 12 2 2 Radio failed both 6 2 2 2 2 2 years; recovered in 75 5 10 5 — 5 Died; radio recovered 4 4 0 1 — 1 Radio failed 12 015 13 1 1 2 1 Radio recovered 54 0.7 44 7 3 6 3 Radio failed in 75. Data included with 1975 Radio recovered in 76 11 2 2 5 5 5 Radio recovered 1 1 — — — — Radio failed 1 1 — — — — Radio failed but recovered 53 2.6 14 7 32 32 12 Radio recovered. With young? 65 0.6 12 9 14 38 2 Radio recovered. With young 7 M A 12 Mar 75 17 Mar 75 08 Apr 75 26 Apr 75 15 F A 01 Jun 75 05 Jun 75 3 F A 19 Jun 75 21 Jun 75 0 V A 03 Aug 75 14 Aug 75 20 M A 31 Aug 75 17 Nov 75 18 Jul 76 20 Jul 76 75 F A 08 Nov 75 19 Nov 75 73 A A 26 Nov 75 26 Nov 75 65 F A 16 Feb 75 16 Feb 75 76 F A 27 Feb 76 12 May 76 19 F A 06 Mar 76 09 May 76 Tabl e 3 (Continued) 98 F A 11 May 76 10 Jun 76 37 22 Jul 76 06 Aug 76 Dat; 21 F A 13 May 76 20 Jul 76 26 42 F A 22 May 76 29 May 76 5 116 M A 09 Jun 76 05 Nov 76 78 37 M YOY 24 Jun 76 01 Sep 76 20 128 F YOY 02 Jul 76 14 Jul 76 5 138 "'7 YOY 17 Jul 76 18 Jul 76 1 148 F YOY 05 Sep 76 18 Sep 76 10 141 M YOY 10 Sep 76 08 Oct 76 21 150 M A 20 Sep 76 25 Sep 76 5 152 F A 13 Oct 76 67+ 110 M YOY 24 Oct 76 09 Nov 76 15 154 M YOY 11 Nov 76 22 Nov 76 5 139 M YOY 16 Nov 76 17 Dec 76 24 156 M YOY 25 Nov 76 28 Nov 76 1 2 2 2 2 1 50 14 9 9 7 16 2 2 2 1 ? 1 'J 2 2 0.5 10 5 7 16 6 Died. Radio Data included in above. recovered. With young 0.6 10 2 10 14 9 Radio recovered. With young? Radio recovered Radio failed. Radio recovered Radio recovered Lost contact Lost contact 1.2 4 4 3 Collar recovered Radio failed? Still operating b d 'j Collar recovered Lost contact 2.1 16 5 2 2 Shot by hunter. Radio recovered Lost contact o 0 1 — — 12 4 4 4 3 1 i 4 4 3 24 13 30+ 30+ 304 8 3 5 3 4 0 1 — 16 5 2 2 2 CO 00 189 Females may move their young from den to den. On 22 May 1976, G. Smith observed an unmarked female carrying her male offspring (No, 109) by the scruff of the neck. This young badger weighed 2 . 3 kg and was capable of considerable mobility on his own. Badgers weighing approximately 2 kg (probably some six weeks old) may venture from the den. As time progresses, the mother spends more time away from the litter. Litters seem to remain together. Radio-tracking information from No. 98 during mid-May revealed that she. was seldom more than a few hundred meters away from her young. Females may carry food to the natal den. Dead Townsend ground squirrels (Spermophilus townsendi) were found at two natal dens by R. Olson and J. Messick on 28 April and 31 May. Messick observed a family group playing "cat and mouse" with a dead ground squirrel on 25 May 1975. While most adult badgers return to previously used dens, the pattern of den use and activity established by radio-equipped females Nos. 19 and 98 is unique to productive females. Individual adult badgers captured on more than one occasion show constant body weights, although size in this species is quite variable. Adult females range from approximately 5.5 to 9.5 kg while males weigh 2 to 5 kg more. Lactating females often loose more than 0.5 kg while raising their litters. Weight and weight gains of young-of-the-year animals are highly variable (Fig. 2), and weight is probably not a reliable indication of age. Our recapture data suggest that young dispersing from their home area may have difficulty maintaining their body weight (Fig. 2). Family groups break up in late May or June, and this coincides with the onset of the breeding season. Most young weigh 4 to 6 kg at this point. Young badgers frequently show wounds obviously made by teeth. This suggests that adults may encourage young to leave their natal area. Wright (1966) suggests that female badgers may sometimes breed during their first summer. While it is not reflected in the raw data (Fig. 2), my impression is that females reach their adult weights faster than males, perhaps supporting Wright's contention that females mature faster than males. Distances moved by young badgers are greater than those of adults. Three young males were killed after leaving their natal area. No. 88 was first trapped at his maternal den on 5 May and weighed 1.6 kg. In early July, a local farmer killed this badger O Fig. 2. Body weights and weight gains of young-of-the-year badgers captured or collected during 1975 and 1976, on or near the Snake River Birds of Prey Natural Area, Idaho, Data were from both marked animals and carcasses. Solid lines connect weights for individuals captured more than once in a given year. 191 after the animal was discovered undermining an irrigation ditch. No. 88 had moved nearly 12.8 km (straight-line). His litter mate, No. 89, weighed 1.8 kg at the maternal den on 1 May. We recaptured him on 3 August, 3.2 km from the initial capture site. He weighed 6.3 kg. A trapper killed this badger on 22 December, 4.4 km from the natal den. His carcass, minus the pelt, weighed 8.3 kg. Directions moved by Nos. 88 and 89 differed by 90°. Badger No. 108 weighed 2 . 3 kg when G. Smith captured him on 22 May. This animal was found dead in the road in early July more than 30 km (straight-line) from the original capture location. This summer and fall, we fitted nine young-of-the-year badgers with radio transmitters designed to expand with growth (Table 3) . In general, young animals move erratically and cover several times the area of an adult's range. Most adults confine movement to 1 km2 (Table 3). The movement pattern of adult male No. 20 (Fig. 3) is typical of non-lactating adult badgers. Contrast this with the movement of male young-of-the-year No. 87 (Fig. 4) . Number 87 weighed 3.2 kg when flooded from his natal den on 29 April. J. whitman trapped this animal on 22 June a few hundred meters from the initial capture site. His weight had increased to 8.3 kg and he was fitted with an expandable radio collar. We monitored No. 87 intermittently until he lost his collar on 1 September. On 3 August this badger was recaptured, and inspection revealed that the collar was causing absolutely no irritation, although his weight had decreased to 6.9 kg. The relatively great distances moved by No. 87 and other young-of-the-year made it difficult for us to regularly locate them. To find missing animals, we resorted to radio-tracking from a Cessna 172. We appreciate the help of T. Dunstan, J. Harper, and Gem State Aviation in this venture. The behavior of certain young-of-the-year badgers contrasts sharply with that of adults. It is uncommon to encounter adult radio-equipped badgers above ground more than perhaps 10 percent of the time. Number 148, a young female, was radio-tracked during September and was often located above ground, even during inclement weather and in daylight. Furthermore, this badger seemed fearless and careless compared with other individuals. We do not know when young badgers and their dispersal and settle into a home area. The movement and activity of males Nos. 139 and 141 during the fall seemed more like that of adults, so it may be that dispersal ends for some young after their first summer. Movement and activity of adult badgers. Distances between capture and recapture sites was seldom greater than 1.5 km, and we 192 N o radio fix &/or den recapture 28 Aug e capture 17 Jun Fig. 3. Movement of adult male badger No. 20, as determined by trapping and radio-tracking, from 31 August through 11 October 1975. 193 ,29Jun radio fix &/or den • capture site with date 1 km N A 5 Aug radio-^ lost S Sei Fig. 4. Movement of male young-of-the-year badger No. 87, as determined by trapping and radio-tracking, from 29 April through 1 September 1976. 194 have trapped representatives of all age and sex classes within a few meters of each other, and in time spans of a few hours. Adult males have even been trapped at active maternal dens. Unrelated adult female No. 155 and young-of-the-year male No. 154 occupied the same den on 22 November 1976. During 1975 and 1976, 27 badgers (12 males and 15 females) were equipped with radio transmitters (Table 3) . Radios operated an average of 5.3 weeks with extremes from a few hours to 22 weeks. In Table 3, I present information on the number of burrows used and reused, maximum time underground, etc. Home areas were subjectively estimated for badgers for which we have ample data. The configuration and movement pattern for adults (Fig. 3) is fairly consistent and has been reported in detail in last year's Annual Report (Messick et al. 1975). Home areas of the same and opposite sexes may overlap. Badgers are more active at dawn, dusk, and during the night, but they are certainly not exclusively nocturnal. Estimated home areas of badgers in this study (Table 3) are smaller than data reported by other investigators. A female radio-tracked in Minnesota for 165 days moved over 2,100 acres (8.4 knn), with more restricted activity during the fall and winter (Sargeant and Warner 1972) . Home ranges of two males monitored in Curlew Valley, Utah and Idaho, averaged 1,440 acres (5.8 km^) , more than twice the size of four females utilizing the same area (Lindzey 1971) . Testes size and condition of females suggest that badgers in this area breed in late May and through June. J. Whitman observed copulation at 1520 on 22 June 1976. Badgers are more active and mobile, especially in the daytime, during the breeding season. Scent glands become obvious, but we do not know if badgers mark with scent, urine, or feces. We do find more scats on the surface during this period. Fighting does occur (observation by G. Smith on 28 June 1976), but we do not know if it regularly happens between males, or is part of copulatory activity. A radio collar lost by female No. 8 on 16 July 1977 (Table 3) was marked by teeth, and we believe that a male was in the vicinity of No. 8 when she lost her radio. My current impression is that badgers have a promiscuous mating system. Badgers are less active during periods of cold weather, although it is not uncommon for badgers to be above ground in blizzards with temperatures below -10°C. J. Messick found fresh badger sign on 9 January 1977 when the temperature was below -22°C. However, radio-tracking data from adult female No. 152 195 indicate that some badgers will remain mostly inactive during the winter. Number 152 has used one burrow continuously since 3 December 1976 with only one recorded emergence (Table 3). In addition, No. 116 (adult male) began remaining underground for longer periods during October and November. Observations of dens known to be occupied by badgers also indicate longer stays underground during the winter than in other seasons. Food, habits, feeding studies and predation. The Townsend ground squirrel is by far the most important item in the badger's diet (Table 4). Seasonal variation in the utilization of ground squirrels does occur, but Table 5 and Fig. 5 demonstrate that badgers are effective predators on hibernating ground squirrels. Scats analyzed for this study were obtained from carcasses collected outside the study area, but in similar habitats, or from badgers confined for 24 or more hours after capture. Some were picked up near dens, etc., but these usually appeared no older than several days, so the chronology in Table 5 and Fig. 5 should be reasonably accurate. Badgers consumed a wide variety of other foods (Tables 4 and 5), but the caloric contribution of alternate prey is minor compared to that of Townsend ground squirrels. Badgers seem to be opportunistic in selecting prey. Scats and stomachs yield only qualitative food habits information. Quantitative consumption rates of free-roaming carnivores that leave no detectable prey remains has not been obtained. Feeding studies under caged conditions can help overcome this void, but it is still necessary to extrapolate from the maintenance diet to account for the animal's activity under wild conditions. Jense (1968) determined that slightly more than 70 g of digestible ground squirrel (Citellus sp.) were required to meet the maintenance requirements of an 8.25 kg female badger. Since he found that ground squirrels contained over 60 percent water and were 90 percent digestible, the 70 g would have to be converted for comparisons with live prey. We have initiated feeding studies in two, 3 by 4.5 m pens. These pens allow limited movement and digging because each contains 1.5 m of soil. Number 155, an adult female, was placed in one of these cages in early December. This animal consumed 2,500 g of mostly Townsend ground squirrels during one month. In an earlier trial, No. 8, also an adult female, used 3,086 g of assorted rodents and reptiles in 15 days. Data from these and other feeding studies, coupled with our radio-tracking information on home range use and activity, should allow us to roughly estimate the biomass of ground squirrels removed by badgers on the Snake River Birds of Prey Natural Area. 196 Table 4. Items encountered in 231 badger scats collected on or near the Snake River Birds of Prey Natural Area, Idaho. Classification No. scats Mammals Aves Townsend ground squirrel (Spermophilus townsendi ) 191 Blacktail jackrabbit (Lepus cal ifornicus) 10 Kangaroo rat (Dipodomys sp.) 12 Deer mouse (Peromyscus maniculatus) 18 Other mammals (identified but not listed here) 10 Unknown mammals 9 All mammals 250 Unknown bird 1 Unknown bird eggs 2 All aves 3 Reptiles Great Basin gopher snake (Pituophis melanoleucus) 7 Collared lizard (Crotaphytus collaris) 4 Whiptail lizard (Cnemidophorus tigris) 3 Side-blotched lizard (Uta stansburiana) 1 Unknown reptiles 6 Reptile eggs 1 1 All reptiles 32 Insects and Arachnids Scorpions 72 Beetles 55 Grasshoppers 22 Other insects and spiders 3 All insects and arachnids 152 197 Table 5. Monthly summary of items encountered in 231 badger scats collected during 1975 and 1976 on or near the Snake River Birds of Prey Natural Area, Idaho.. Individual species within each taxonomic grouping d.re given in Table 4. -a to r— -1 — (13 CO ■p ooho T3 CO -a rO SZ C CD CO -i- U , — ! ■ +-> o> oj 1 — +J c jr CO 2 n3 Q. CO CO ■r— o x: -p E E 3 3 a> CL CO rd o O i — rd re X o • O > cu c: s- ^ -z. oo|+-> <=c a: i—i ro Jan 0 -- -- -- — -- -- Feb 6 6 3 3 0 0 0 Mar 7 6 3 3 0 0 0 Apr 6 5 2 3 0 0 0 May 23 24 8 16 0 2 5 Jun 47 48 n 37 0 7 18 Jul 99 94 17 77 1 9 39 Aug 17 19 7 12 0 3 18 Sep 14 9 2 7 1 0 18 Oct 4 5 2 3 0 0 6 Nov 5 5 1 4 0 0 3 Dec 3 1 0 1 0 1 0 19! 20 % No Samples M m 23 47 99 17 14 l; Fig. 5. Monthly summary of the percentage of badger scats containing Townsend ground squirrels (Spermophilus townsendi) . The total number of scats analyzed for each month appears to the right of the bars. 199 With Townsend ground squirrels so common in the diet of badgers, we wonder if badger predation influences the density of this prey. We considered devising a predator or prey removal experiment to answer this question. However, it does not seem worthwhile to approach the problem in this way for several reasons. Perhaps central to this argument is that Townsend ground squirrel populations are probably regulated by a mix of intrinsic and extrinsic factors. Under the constraints of limited time and manpower, any attempt to sort out the relative importance of avian and coyote, versus badger predation would probably end in failure. Complicating this are various intrinsic population control mechanisms such as disease, dispersal, and physiological changes in natality. Predation is a density dependent process, so that any results from one area with a given population of badgers and squirrels might not be valid for other habitats with different numbers of predators and prey. D. Johnson, G. Smith, and the other workers on Study V have recorded badger hunting activity on their 1 ha grids. This information, pooled with data on badger food habits, home range use, activity, etc., may allow us to at least describe the qualitative aspects of badger predation on ground squirrels. Furthermore, we might be able to design a useful experiment for future testing which would determine the effects badgers exert on populations of Townsend ground squirrels. The basic variables of density, sex and age structure, natality, and mortality we are delineating for the badger population must be the initial step in any population study. These data are essential before any hypotheses about predation or other regulatory mechanisms can be tested. Other activities: Monthly carcass collections now total 115 badgers. These reproductive tracts, stomach samples, and teeth (for aging) have only been partially studied. Blood samples are regularly taken from badgers and tested for antibodies to plague (Yersinia pestis) . D. Johnson and G. Smith report these results in their Annual Report for Study V. J. Messick gave presentations on this project to elementary and junior high students at Melba, Idaho, and to some of the biology classes at Borah High School, Boise, Idaho. R. Olson, J. Whitman, and J. Messick discussed this work with several different groups associated with, or sponsored by the BLM. In late September, J. Messick met with his research committee at the University of British Columbia. 200 Other Predators Coyote food habits: Scats were collected from carcasses, or picked up along roads and trails on and near the Snake River Birds of Prey Natural Area. These reveal that jackrabbits (Lepus californicus) were most important in the diet of coyotes from a biomass standpoint (Table 6). Townsend ground squirrels would only be available during part of the year. Squirrels are probably valuable to young coyotes and females with litters. Analysis of the collection of coyote scats is incomplete, and most of the samples in Table 5 were acquired from April to October. Carcass collections: Monthly carcass collections, a rich source of age, reproductive, and dietary information, now total 73 coyotes and five bobcats. We have also collected 173 coyote and five bobcat scats. Analysis is only partially complete. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS We thank L. Guilliams, H. Fannon, K. Leavitt, J. Amos, J. Gahl, J. and S. Cowger, L. Noe, S. Greenfield, the E.T. Evans family, K. Brant, and many of the contractors and staff on the Snake River Birds of Prey Research Project for help with carcass collections, tag recoveries, etc. LITERATURE CITED Crowe, D.M. 1975. Population structure of some mammalian predators in southeastern Wyoming. J. Wildl. Manage 39(2):449-450. Jense, G.K. 1968. Food habits and energy utilization of badgers. M.S. Thesis, South Dakota State Univ., Brookings. 39 pp. Lindzey, F.G. 1971. Ecology of badgers in Curlew Valley, Utah and Idaho with emphasis on movement and activity patterns. M.S. Thesis, Utah State Univ., Logan. 50 pp. Messick, J. P., M.G. Hornocker, I.T.McT. Cowan and J. Whitman. 1975. Dynamics of predation upon a raptor prey base in the Snake River Birds of Prey Natural Area pages 136-154 In Snake River Birds of Prey Res. Proj . Ann. Rep., 1975. U.S.- Dept. of Interior, Bureau of Land Management, Boise, Idaho. 193 pp. Sargeant, A.B. and D.W. Warner. 1972. Movements and denning habits of a badger. J. Mammal. 53 (1) : 207-210. Wright, P.L. 1966. Observations on the reproductive cycle of the American badger (Taxidea taxus) . Comparative biology of reproduction in mammals. Symposia, of the Zool. Soc. London. 15:37-45. 201 Table 6. Items encountered in 46 coyote scats collected during 1975 and 1976 on or near the Snake River Birds of Prey Natural Area, Idaho. Classification No. scats Mammals Blacktail jackrabbit (Lepus call fornicus) 21 Townsend ground squirrel [Spermopni lus" townsendi) 22 Whitefooted mice (Peromyscus sp.) 15 Kangaroo rat (Dipodomys sp.) 4 Mountain cottontail (Sylvilagus nuttali) 3 Woodrat (Neotoma sp.) 2 Townsend pocket gopher (Thomomys townsendi ) 1 American badger (Taxidea taxusj 1 Unknown vole (Microtus sp. ) 1 Unknown lagomorph 2 Domestic cow 1 All mammals 73 Aves Raven (Corvus corax) 1 Unknown passerine 1 All aves 2 Reptiles Unknown lizard 1 All reptiles 1 Insects and arachnids Beetles 49 Grasshoppers 43 Scorpions 6 All insects and arachnids 98 Other Various vegetation 24 202 r I Graham Smith, Study V, records information from one of 472 Townsend ground squirrels trapped in 1976 (Photo by L. Oftedahl) STUDY V: CONTRACTOR: INVESTIGATORS: PROJECT SUPPORT: OBJECTIVES: 203 Population ecology and habitat requirements of Townsend ground squirrels. Department of Biological Sciences, University of Idaho, Moscow, Idaho 83843. Donald R. Johnson, Principal Investigator. Graham W. Smith, Research Associate. Randall M. Olson, Research Aid. U.S. Department of Interior, Bureau of Land Management, Contract No. 52500-CT5-1002. 1. Describe population variables of ground squirrel populations including productivity, recruitment, age and sex ratios, and turnover rates. 2. Ascertain population density (a) in representative habitat types, (b) in relation to land use patterns including the effects of cultivation on density, and, (c) in hunting areas of those raptors outlined by the radio-telemetry study (Study III). 3. Ascertain food habits and habitat requirements in the major vegetation types. 4. Isolate factors (predation, disease, parasites, etc.) which may limit ground squirrel density and distribution. ANNUAL SUMMARY Populations of Townsend ground squirrels (Spermophilus towns end i) were studied from January through July, 1976, the second year of a four year study of squirrel ecology and habitat requirements. Squirrels occurred at peak densities in vegetation types disturbed by fire and heavy grazing where annual grasses had replaced native vegetation. Densities of resident squirrels (adults and yearlings) ranged from 8 to 20/ha. The overall sex ratio was balanced but that of adults and yearlings favored females, indicating a higher mortality in males. Over winter survivorship of juvenile females exceeded that of the adults of either sex. Litters at birth averaged 7.7 young and at emergence 5.2 young per female. All adult females bred, and at least 62 percent of the yearling females produced litters. Growth rates, based on weight changes of free-living animals, were calculated. Adult males entered torpor in late May and early June, adult females throughout June and juveniles from mid-June to mid-July. In early spring, squirrels utilized green forage, primarily the leaves of cheatgrass. Forb seeds and the leaves of shadscale 204 and winterfat comprised most of the diet later in the activity season. Six squirrels have been found positive for plague (Yersinia pestis) within the study area. Badgers show elevated titers of plague antibodies indicating intermittent exposure. Prairie falcons, badgers and rattlesnakes exert a sustained predation pressure on these ground squirrel populations, particu- larly the young-of-the-year . INTRODUCTION The Townsend ground squirrel is the primary prey item of prairie falcons (Ogden 1973), red-tailed hawks and ravens (Kochert 1973) which nest within the Birds of Prey Natural Area (BPNA) . Ground squirrels comprise a minor portion of the diet of golden eagles (Beecham 1970; Kochert 1972, 1973). Badgers, which occur in large numbers within the BPNA, and rattlesnakes also prey heavily on ground squirrels. Recognizing the importance of the Townsend ground squirrel in this ecosystem, the Bureau of Land Management has contracted a four year study of its ecology. STUDY AREA Five intensive study sites were established in 1975, and two additional sites were established in 1976. These sites are representative of the vegetation types found within the BPNA (Fig. 1). Each site is 1 ha in area (100 x 100 m) with grid stakes at 10 m intervals. Tables la and lb summarize the plant coverage at these sites for 1975 and 1976. The intensive study sites are: 1. Bedspring Site. This site is used yearly as a sheep camp during the winter grazing season and as a result is richly fertilized with droppings. Its vegetation consists primarily of a dense lawn of cheatgrass (Bromus tectorum) . 2. Eurotia Site. This site supports a stand of winterfat (Eurotia lanata) with an annual fescue (Festuca octoflora) and cheatgrass understory. 3. Burn Site. This site was burned during a range fire 24 June 1974. Its vegetation consists primarily of cheatgrass and Sandberg bluegrass (Poa sandbergii) . Before the burn this site supported a vegetation similar to the Comparison Site. 4. Comparison Site. This site is dominated by big sage- brush (Artemisia tridentata) , hop sage (Atriplex spinosa) and winterfat. Sandberg bluegrass occurs as an understory. 205 Intensive Study Sites Fig. 1. Location of intensive sites in the Snake River Birds of Prey Study Area. Table la. Plant coverage [%) , May, 1975 and 1976, on intensive sites, Taxa with coverage less than 1% excluded. Winterfat Healthy 1975 1976 (Eurotia) Shrubs Eurotia lanata Artemisia tridentata Atriplex spinosa lit. 5 12.2 Depleted 1975 1976 Bedspring) <1 1.3 Shrub-grass Unburned 1975 1976 (Comparison) 9.8 8,5 5-i U.3 2.8 l.l Burned 1975 1976 ( Burn ) <1 <1 Forbs w Cruciferae <1 _ 45.7 cm from the front of the camera box. It was preferable to have the scrape small enough so feeding activity is localized and the young cannot move out of the field of view. Also, we attempted to locate scrapes with different numbers of young. The above requirements required checking many scrapes before we selected four that would be acceptable for camera placement. We also selected four other nests for comparison checks on growth of young. All selected scrapes were checked every four days to determine the exact hatching date, to measure the growth rate of the young, and to collect prey remains. After hatching, the cameras were inserted in four scrapes containing 2, 3, 5 and 7 young. All selected nest were entered every three or four days to change film, collect prey remains and measure young. On the first visit to a scrape after hatching, we individually marked each nestling by coloring the down on top of the head with a felt-tip marking pen. This technique worked 229 well with only periodic touch-ups needed for fast and easy identi- fication of the young. We continued using this technique after the young were banded to aid identification and minimize the time for nest visits. After hatching and camera placement, we conducted dawn-to- dusk observations on selected camera scrapes. The one camera we compared with intensive observations was remotely triggered during each feeding visit. We set all other cameras to take one frame every thirty seconds during daylight hours. Most observations on one of the camera scrapes were correlated with personnel from Study III since both the adult birds were instrumented with radios. In this way we had a check on the number of feeding visits, type of habitat utilized to secure prey, and the density of prey in feeding areas. During our dawn-to-dusk observations, we noted all feeding visits, the behavior associated with feeding, and any aggression displayed with prairie falcons in adjacent territories. As the season progressed, we conducted cliff searches to locate dead or dying young prairie falcons. This was especially important to determine types and causes of mortality during and after fledging. Just prior to fledging, we placed radio packages on six nestlings from four different scrapes. These radio packages were described by Dunstan (Study III) in the 1975 annual report. We used these birds to record movement of fledged young prior to dispersal. Study III personnel followed these radioed young during dispersal from the canyon. Our field work ceased when all young from the selected scrapes had dispersed or disappeared. After the field season, we obtained two rehabilitated adult prairie falcons from Morley Nelson to use in a feeding study. This study was designed to compare remains in pellets with known types of food fed to the falcons at specific intervals. The prey items are weighed before feeding and each day the pellets and prey remains are collected, described and weighed. Daily weight changes of the birds are recorded. Once each week all excrements and molted feathers are collected and weighed. The prey items are segregated so that a diet of mammals will be fed for a certain period, followed by a diet of exclusively avian prey. In this way, we can see what effects each diet has on consumption rates, wastage, and weight of the birds. RESULTS Raptor Density Our intensive study area, a 7 km portion of the BPNA near Swan Falls Dam, has a high density of nesting prairie falcons. 230 We surveyed only the north side of the river and found 21 nesting pairs of prairie falcons. This area also included four raven (Corvus corax) nests, one red-tailed hawk (Buteo jamaicensis) , one great horned owl (Bubo virginianus) , one kestrel (Falco sparverius) , and one turkey vulture (Cathartes aura) . The vulture nest was the first to be located within the BPNA. Cliff structure and placement of scrapes indicated breeding pairs at scrapes had limited or no visibility of adjacent scrapes due to canyon wall projections or the stepped nature of the wall. These projections are obvious to the human observer when viewed from the rim or from beneath the scrape. In one area of high cliffs, a staggered system of high- low scrape locations appears to help shorten the average distance between scrapes. In most cases, both scrapes and favorite perches had a limited view of an adjacent bird's territory. Assessment of Film Records Forty-eight rolls (2,400 feet) of film were taken in four different camera scrapes. Only 40 rolls contained any information at all. Even these rolls were plagued by large portions of over- exposed or blank portions of film. This failure was attributed to the temperature extremes characteristic of the exposed cliffs in this area and failure of the camera's automatic metering system. We are still working on these films and will incorporate this information in a later report, but the data contained in these records appears to be of limited value. We should be able to extract information on prey items used. Prey Caching Last year we observed prey caching in 16 of 84 feeding visits (19 percent) . We noted additional incidences this year among seven different nesting pairs. This behavior does not appear to be isolated to certain individuals or depend on the number of young in the scrape. One pair which had two young last year continued to cache prey even though they had seven young this year. One adult female cached prey immediately after taking it from the male and then returned to the same cached item six hours later and consumed it. This particular prey item was never taken to the scrape. Food Habits Analysis of periodic pellet and prey item collections revealed 12 species of mammals, birds and reptiles (Table 1). Townsend ground squirrels were the most common prey item, both in total number identified and percentage of occurrence (Table 2). When these data are segregated by two week intervals, there is an increasing percent occurrence of birds and lizards through the nesting period. Personnel from Study V (D. Johnson, pers. comm. ) documented a weight loss in Townsend ground squirrels during 231 Table 1. Prey items observed in pellets or remains found in prairie falcon scrapes. Common Name Scientific Name MAMMALS Townsend Ground Squirrel White-tailed Antelope Ground Squirrel Coyote Spermophilus townsendi Ammospermophilus leucurus Canis latrans BIRDS Horned Lark Western Meadowlark Sage Thrasher Cliff Swallow House Finch Chukar Erpmophila alpestris S1 urnella neglecta Oreoscoptes montanus Petrochelidon pyrrhonota Carpoda cus mexicanus Alectoris graeca LIZARDS Desert Horned Lizard Western Whiptail Lizard Leopard Lizard Phrynosoma platyrhinos Cnemidophorus tigris Crotaphytus wislizenii 232 Table 2. Number and percent frequency of mammals, birds and lizards in pellets and prey remains found in prairie falcon scrapes, 1976. * Total Total 1-15 16-31 1-15 16-30 Collec- Indiv. May May June June tions Ident. No. of Collections 20 24 24 9 77 Collection Containing Townsend Ground Squirrels 20 24 24 8 76 105 Collections Containing Other Mammals % Frequency for Mammals Collections Containing Birds % Frequency for Birds Collections Containing Lizards % Frequency for Lizards Data from six intensively studied scrapes plus a few collections from other scrapes visited for incidental reasons. 0 0 0 1 1 100 100 100 89 99 1 5 11 4 21 5 28 46 44 27 0 1 3 3 7 0 4 13 33 9 233 June. This was attributed to inclement weather which reduced the foraging time of the ground squirrels. The increase in avian and reptilian prey suggest: (1) decreased vulnerability of the Town- send ground squirrels because they were underground, and (2) the importance of birds and lizards as prey items for fledged prairie falcons. This trend is not apparent in individual pairs. One pair fed exclusively on Townsend ground squirrels and another pair fed on birds in early June, but then fed mainly on ground squirrels later in the nesting season. It was interesting to record the white-tailed antelope ground squirrel in the prey collections. This means either the birds are crossing the river canyon to hunt, or antelope ground squirrels are present on the north rim of the canyon. Lowell Diller (Study IV, pers. comm. ) found antelope ground squirrels in rattlesnake scats on the north rim. A piece of coyote skin was found in one scrape. This incidence may be the result of wood rat activity, carrion eating by the falcon, or predation on a young coyote pup. Aggression Prairie falcons are very tenacious when defending their territories against other intruding prairie falcons or other raptor species. One or both of the territorial pair will attack the intruder with repeated stoops and vocalizations until the intruder is completely out of the territory. This tenacity lessens as the nesting season progresses. The last territorial aggression involving contact between adjacent pairs that we observed in 1976 occurred on 20 May, two weeks before young from one nest fledged. Defenses shortly before and after this time were limited to brief chase flights or strong vocalizations. Territorial aggression was renewed when young fledged from adjacent territories. At this time all aggression was displayed against young from adjacent territories with almost complete tolerance of other adults. This aggression was strong and often the contact was audible. Adults extended their territories up to 200 m from the cliff face to attack young, and several times were joined by additional adults in attacking young birds. The young birds were attacked repeatedly until driven off or forced to land. On several occasions these attacks resulted in such a hard blow to the young bird that it dropped several meters before recovering. On 13 June, a tourist witnessed an attack which actually killed the young bird. Examination of the dead bird revealed a crushed skull. During our cliff searches, we found two other birds which also had crushed skulls. Their condition and position in sage clumps or on cliffs indicated they also were victims of this type of aggression. Cliff searches also revealed seven other fledglings that had been partially consumed by some predator. It is possible 234 that some of these birds may have been killed by aggression and later scavenged by another predator. First flights of young birds are unpredictable, and erratic. When the population density is high, these flights may take them into adjacent territories. The aggression related mortality may, therefore, depend on the density of the population in that partic- ular area. Radio-tracking of young birds showed a definite affinity to their home area. This may be the result of repeated attacks in adjacent territories. Other Mortality In conducting this aspect of the study, we entered scrapes both before and after hatching to document the disappearance of eggs or young during hatching. Loss of eggs or young occurred in two out of six scrapes (total of three eggs lost) which we had visited shortly before and after hatching. The elapsed time between visits was 3 to 5 days. Infertile eggs may have broken, but it is quite common to find unhatched or infertile eggs still in the scrape even as late as fledging. The simultaneous breaking of these eggs at hatching seems unlikely. In 1975, we found one dead day old prairie falcon chick in a cache. In 1976, we docu- mented a scrape that had five eggs before 10 May, but on that date it contained one young and two eggs in the process of hatching. There was no sign of the other eggs or young. Some chips from hatched eggs were present, but we were unable to determine which eggs they came from or how many eggs they represented. The size and shape of the scrape prevented any accidental expulsion. No evidence of eggs or young were present beneath the scrape. It is possible that the other two young died during hatching, were carried from the nest, and deposited in a cache similar to that recorded in 1975. Additional mortality occurred later in the nesting cycle. This mortality occurred while the young were still downy, but large enough to move around the scrape. These individuals were usually found beneath the scrape. Fratricide would be a logical explanation, except in cases where the entire clutch disappears. Similar cases were noted in two different scrapes this year. Both of these scrapes had heavy infestations of swallow bedbugs (Oeciacus vicarius) . In these scrapes there may have been a voluntary abandonment of the scrape by the young falcons. Such abandonment may not necessarily result in death. On two occasions, we found young that had disappeared from scrapes but were being fed and protected on the ground by the adults. In one case, the young had been outside the scrape for 12 to 17 days. In both cases, we observed feeding visits or found prey remains with the vagrant young even though as many as six other young remained in the original scrapes. 235 Our cliff searches were conducted along 5.6 km of cliff which contained 14 nesting pairs. If we assume 3.34 young fledge per attempt, as we found for the entire BPNA in 1975, then the ten dead juveniles found represent 21 percent of the total fledglings. Ten of 17 attempts (59 percent) were monitored sufficiently to determine a fledging rate of 2.7 young per attempt. If this is expanded to the four nests of unknown success, then post-fledging mortality could be 26 percent. Growth Comparisons During the year we measured the growth of young falcons to see if the number of young in a scrape affected development or fledging time. Clutch sizes of study scrapes varied from 2 to 7 young per scrape. There was no significant difference in fledging times of these scrapes (Table 3) . Weight was correlated with age (r > 0.98; Figures 1 and 2). All known age birds were plotted for the first 25 days at which time maximum weight was attained. The birds were from two different clutch sizes and each sex was plotted separately. Individual birds often lost weight before fledging. Wing length was correlated with age after 20 days (r > 0.97; Figures 3 and 4). This wing length was measured from the notch in the wrist joint (antebrachium and manus) in a straight line distance to the end of the 8th primary. Feeding Study This part of our study is still in progress and a more complete analysis will be reported later. For the past six months we have been feeding two captive prairie falcons selected prey items collected on or near the Birds of Prey Natural Area. In this way we can see what parts are eaten and what items appear in the pellets. At the same time, we are conducting some basic bioener- getics studies. 236 Table 3. Fledging dates of clutches with different numbers of young. Scrape No . Y oung Days to Name Per S crape Fledging Camera 1 7 38-41 Dam Rd. 5 35-40 Black Rock 3 36-38 B.B. Powerl ine 2 38-40 237 800- 700- 600- _j FROM CLUTCH OF 5 = - FROM CLUTCH OF 3 r = 0.99 CO < CD 500- — 400- I- X C£ UJ 300- 200- 100- 1 1 1 10 15 20 AGE IN DAYS t 25 - 1- 30 Fig. 1. Age in days vs. weight in juvenile female prairie falcons . 238 • " ? FROM CLUTCH OF 5 600' ■ "2 FROM CLUTCH OF 3 # r = 0.98 • / y' 0 o 0 • w> 500- 2 < • / O tr (3 400' ■ ° / • / ■z. /v t- X 2 300- uu /a