Skip to main content

Full text of "Animal keepers' forum"

See other formats


Oeiiicaieil  Issue  on 
ftirian  Husbandry 


BII/MAV  2 


ANIMAL  KEEPERS’  FORUM.  3601  S.W.  29th  St.,  Suite  133,  Topeka,  KS  66614-2054 
Phone:  (785)  273-9149  FAX  (785)  273-1980 


April/May  2010 
Vol.37,  No.  4/5 


AKF  Managing  Editor:  Susan  D.  Chan  • Associate  Editors:  Becky  Richendollar,  North  Carolina  Zoo  • Mark  de 
Denus,  Winnipeg,  MB  • Enrichment  Options  Column  Coordinator:  Julie  Hartell-DeNardo,  Oakland  Zoo  and  Ric 
Kotarsky,  Tulsa  Zoo  & Living  Museum  • Legislative/Conservation  Outlook  Column  Co-Coordinators:  Becky 
Richendollar,  North  Carolina  Zoo  and  Greg  McKinney,  Philadelphia,  PA  • Reactions  Column  Coordinator: 
William  K.  Baker,  Jr.,  Abilene  Zoo,  Abilene,  TX  • ATC  Column  Co-Cordinators:  Angela  Binney,  Disney’s  Animal 
Kingdom;  Kim  Kezer,  Zoo  New  England;  Jay  Pratte,  Zoo  Atlanta  • Conservation  Station  Coordinator:  Amanda 
Kamradt,  New  England  AAZK  Chapter  • Proofreader:  Barbara  Manspeaker,  AAZK  Administrative  Office. 

Animal  Keepers’  Forum  is  published  monthly  by  the  American  Association  of  Zoo  Keepers,  Inc.,  3601  S.W.  29th 
Street,  Suite  133,  Topeka,  KS  66614-2054.  Ten  dollars  of  each  membership  fee  goes  toward  the  annual  publica- 
tion costs  of  Animal  Keepers’  Forum  . Postage  paid  at  Topeka,  KS. 

AAZK  Executive  Director:  Ed  Hansen,  AAZK,  Inc.,  Topeka  KS 

also  serves  as  AAZK  Liaison  to  the  American  Zoo  & Aquarium  Association  (AZA) 

AAZK  Administrative  Secretary:  Barbara  Manspeaker,  AAZK,  Inc.,  Topeka,  KS 

BOARD  OF  DIRECTORS 

Shane  Good,  Cleveland  Metroparks  Zoo,  Cleveland,  OH  44109  President 

Bob  Cisneros,  San  Diego  Zoo,  San  Diego,  CA  92112-055 1 Vice  President 

Tammy  Root,  Indianapolis  Zoo,  Indianapolis,  IN  46222 

Gisela  Wiggins,  North  Carolina  Zoo,  Asheboro,  NC  27205-1425 

Penny  Jolly,  Disney’s  Animal  Kingdom,  Lake  Buena  Vista,  FL  32830-1000 


COMMITTEES/COORDINATORS/PR0.1ECT  MANAGERS 


By-laws 

Chair  - Gisela  A.  Wiggins,  North  Carolina  Zoo 

Grants  Committee 

Chair  - Shelly  Roach,  Columbus  Zoo 

Behavioral  Husbandry  Committee 

Chair  - Deana  Walz,  The  Living  Planet  Aquarium 

Professional  Development 

Melaina  Wallace,  Disney’s  Animal  Kingdom 

ICZ  Coordinators 

Shane  Good,  Cleveland  Metroparks  Zoo 
and  Norah  Farnham,  Woodland  Park  Zoo 
Media/Website 

Denise  Wagner,  The  Phoenix  Zoo,  Project  Manager 


Ethics 

Chair  - Bob  Cisneros,  San  Diego  Zoo 
Awards 

Chair  - Janet  McCoy,  The  Oregon  Zoo 
Marketing  Resources 

Annual  Conferences 

Coordinator  - Ed  Hansen,  AAZK,  Inc. 

Conservation  Committee  Chair 

Amanda  Kamradt,  New  England  AAZK  Chapter 

Bowling  for  Rhinos 

Coordinator  - Patty  Pearthree,  Cary,  NC 

Products  and  Membership 

Jacque  Blessington,  K.C.  Zoo,  Project  Manager 


Conferences 


Pat  Owen.  Woodland  Park  Zoo.  Project  Manager 


MEMBERSHIP  SERVICES 

Data  Transfer  Forms 

Available  for  download  at  www.aazk.org 


AAZK  Publicatiions/Logo  Products 

AAZK  Administrative  Office/Topeka 


pr  inted  on  Recycled  Paper 


Table  of  Contents 

2010  AAZK  Conference  Notice/Information  for  Contributors... 131 

About  the  Cover.......................... 132 

From  the  President/From  the  AKF  Editor.... 133 

Learning  to  Walk  Again:  Paralysis  in  an  African  Penguin .......134  - 138 

Development  of  an  Aviary  Training  Program:  Enhancing  the  Daily  Management 

of  Zoo  Atlanta’s  Bird  Collection.. 140  - 144 

Hand-Rearing  Caribbean  & Chilean  Flamingo  Chicks  at  the  Birmingham  Zoo 145  - 148 

Breeding  Cinereous  Vultures  at  the  Milwaukee  County  Zoo... 149  - 153 

The  Use  of  Vocal  Playback  as  a Tool  for  Breeding  in  Captive  Birds  - Collaborators  Sought.... 153 

Avian  Rearing  Resource  Website 154 

Designing  a Keeper  and  Bird-Friendly  Enrichment  Program 155  - 157 

Successfully  Raising  Parent-reared  Chicks  from  Elegant-crested  Tinamou... ..158  - 160 

Ratite  Behavioral  Husbandry  at  the  Philadelphia  Zoo ..161  - 163 

Frogmouth  Management  at  Sea  World  Orlando  and  Discovery  Cove 164  - 168 

Incubation  and  Hand-rearing  of  a Greater  Rhea  at  the  Great  Plains  Zoo........... 169  - 1 72 

First  Time  Breeding  of  Captive  Sulawesi  Knobbed  Hombills 

at  St.  Augustine  Alligator  Farm  Zoological  Park 173  - 175 

Artificially  Inseminating  White-naped  Cranes.. .....176  - 182 

Enrichment  and  Training... It’s  For  the  Birds.. 183  - 189 

Jess  Free... 190  - 192 

Greater  Flamingo  with  Capture  Myopathy:  Specialized  Care  and  Husbandry 193  - 195 

Successful  Rearing  of  an  African  Sacred  Ibis  Chick  at  the  Toronto  Zoo ......196  - 200 

Feather  Facts 200 

Semen  Collectiion  on  Rockhopper  and  Gentoo  Penguins  at  the  Indianapolis  Zoo 201  - 205 


Setting  Up  a Picture  Perfect  Success 206  - 208 

Supplemental  Feedings  to  Aid  a Parent-reared  African  Pygmy  Falcon  Chick 209  - 212 

Breeding  White-crested  Laughing  Thrush  at  the  Birmingham  Zoo 213  - 219 

Types  of  Feathers ....219 

The  Management  and  Breeding  Program  for  Roseate  Spoonbills  at 

the  Palm  Beach  Zoo  at  Dreher  Park 220  - 224 

Feather  Types  - Did  You  Know?... 224 

Puppets:  Construction  and  Use  for  the  Hand-rearing  of  Birds .....225  - 230 

Raising  Red- Vented  Bulbuls  in  Tropic  World  Asia  Bird  Holding 231  - 233 

Sessions  for  All  Seasons:  Lappet-faced  Vulture  Training 234  - 237 

Avian  Trivia .237 

Successfully  Managing  a Mixed-Species  Free-Flight  Aviary 238  - 246 

Book  Review  {On  the  Wings  of  Cranes) 247 

Avian  Websites  to  Check  Out 247 

African  Penguin  (Photo) 248 


36th  Anniversary  - 1974  - 2010 

MISSION  STATEMENT 
(Revised  April  2009) 

American  Association  of  Zoo  Keepers,  Inc. 


The  mission  of  the  American  Association  of  Zoo  Keepers,  Inc.  is  to  advance  excellence  in  the  animal 
keeping  profession,  foster  effective  communcation  beneficial  to  animal  care,  support  deserving 
conservation  projects,  and  promote  the  preservation  of  our  natural  resources  and  animal  life. 


ATTENTION  !!! 

Registration  is  now  open  for  the  2010  National 
AAZK  Conference  in  Philadelphia!  This  year’s 
conference  is  August  22-26,  2010.  To  ensure 
that  you  get  the  early  registration  price,  you  must 
register  by  June  22,  2010!!  To  register,  please 
visit  our  website,  www.philadelphiaaazk.org  and 
click  on  “2010  National  Conference.”  Delegates 
may  purchase  either  a full-week  or  a single-day 
registration.  Full-week  registration  for  AAZK, 

ABMA  and  ICZ  members  is  $195  and  for  non- 
members is  $250.  After  June  22,  2010,  there  will 
be  an  additional  $50  late  registration  fee. 

Articles  sent  to  Animal  Keepers ' Forum  will  be  reviewed  by  the  editorial  staff  for  publication.  Articles  of  a 
research  or  technical  nature  will  be  submitted  to  one  or  more  of  the  zoo  professionals  who  serve  as  referees 
for  AKF.  No  commitment  is  made  to  the  author,  but  an  effort  will  be  made  to  publish  articles  as  soon  as 
possible.  Lengthy  articles  may  be  separated  into  monthly  installments  at  the  discretion  of  the  editor.  The 
editor  reserves  the  right  to  edit  material  without  consultation  unless  approval  is  requested  in  writing  by 
the  author.  Materials  submitted  will  not  be  returned  unless  accompanied  by  a stamped,  self-addressed, 
appropriately-sized  envelope.  Telephone,  fax  or  email  contributions  of  late-breaking  news  or  last-minute 
insertions  are  accepted  as  space  allows.  Phone  785-273-9149;  FAX  (785)  273-1980;  email  is  akfeditor@ 
zk.kscoxmail.com<  If  you  have  questions  about  submission  guidelines,  please  contact  the  Editor. 

Deadline  for  each  regular  issue  is  the  10th  of  the  preceding  month. 

Dedicated  issues  may  have  separate  deadline  dates  and  will  be  noted  by  the  editor. 

Articles  printed  do  not  necessarily  reflect  the  opinions  of  the  AKF  staff  or  the  American  Association  of  Zoo 
Keepers,  Inc.  Publication  does  not  indicate  endorsement  by  the  Association. 

Items  in  this  publication  may  be  reprinted  providing  credit  to  this  publication  is  given  and  a copy  of  the 
reprinted  material  is  forwarded  to  the  editor.  If  an  article  is  shown  to  be  separately  copyrighted  by  the 
author(s),  then  permission  must  be  sought  from  the  author(s).  Reprints  of  material  appearing  in  this  journal 
may  be  ordered  from  the  editor.  Regular  back  issues  are  available  for  $4.00  each.  Special  issues  may  cost 
more. 


E-Mail  Addresses: 

You  may  reach  Barbara  Manspeaker  at  AAZK  Administrative  Offices  at:  aazkoffice@zk.kscoxmail.com 
You  may  reach  Susan  Chan  and  Animal  Keepers'  Forum  at:  akfeditor@zk.kscoxmail.com 

Mailing  Address: 

AAZK,  Inc., 

3601  SW  29th  St,  Suite  133 
Topeka,  KS  66614-2054 


AAZK  website  Address:  www.aazk.org 
BFR  Website:  http://aazkbfr.org 


Animal  Keepers'  Forum,  VoL  37  No.  4/5  131 


About  the  Cover. 


This  month’s  cover  features  a Red-tailed  Hawk  (Buteo  jamaicensis)  photographed  by  Karl  Rebenstorf, 
an  animal  care  volunteer  for  the  Virginia  Living  Museum  in  Newport  News,  VA.  This  particular  bird 
is  cared  for  and  utilized  by  the  staff  and  volunteers  to  educate  students  and  public  visitors.  It  came 
from  the  Wildlife  Waystation  of  Sylmar,  CA  in  1993  and  although  the  cause  of  injury  is  not  known, 
this  hawk  is  blind  in  one  eye  and  unable  to  hunt,  and  therefore  not  a candidate  for  release.  When  a 
rehabilitation  organization  receives  an  injured  Red-tailed  Hawk,  there  are  three  most  likely  causes. 
The  first  is  vehicular  collision,  likely  due  to  the  bird  chasing  its  prey  into  traffic.  The  other  two  are 
shooting  and  accidental  trapping. 

Sometimes  referred  to  in  the  United  States  as  a “chickenhawk,”  it  is  one  of  the  most  common  buteos 
throughout  the  North  American  Continent.  Red-tailed  Hawks  can  be  found  in  just  about  every  type 
of  open  habitat  including  deserts,  grasslands,  forests,  pastures,  parks,  and  urban  areas.  They  feed 
primarily  on  small  mammals  such  as  mice,  voles,  rats,  rabbits,  snowshoe  hares,  and  ground  squirrels, 
but  as  an  opportunistic  feeder,  their  diet  may  also  include  birds,  snakes,  lizards,  and  carrion. 

A male  Red-tailed  Hawk  can  weigh  from  approximately  1.56-2.875  lbs.  [~708-1304g]  and  measure 
18-22  in.  long  [~  46-55cm].  Females,  displaying  sexual  dimorphism  in  size,  can  weigh  from 
approximately  2-3.25  lbs.  [~  907-1475g]  and  measure  19-26  in.  long  [~  48-66cm],  up  to  25%  larger 
than  the  males.  Both  have  a wingspan  of  between  45-52  in.  [~  1.1 43- 1.32m]  and  a broad  tail  that 
acquires  its  characteristic  red  color  at  about  one  year  of  age,  after  their  first  molt.  They  are  the  second 
largest  buteo  hawk  in  North  America  after  the  Ferruginous  Hawk  {Buteo  regalis). 

A mated  pair  will  remain  together  in  the  same  nesting  territory  for  years  and  only  take  a new  mate  if 
one  of  the  original  pair  dies.  When  courting,  they  perform  an  impressive  aerial  display  in  which  they 
soar  in  wide  circles  at  first.  Then  the  male  repeats  a pattern  of  several  steep  dives  and  climbs  before 
approaching  the  female  when  they  briefly  touch,  sometimes  grabbing  onto  each  other  and  falling  in 
spirals  toward  the  ground  before  they  separate. 

Red-tailed  Hawks  typically  will  not  begin  to  breed  until  three  years  of  age.  A pair  will  construct  a 
new  nest  or  refurbish  an  old  one.  The  nests  are  comprised  of  piles  of  sticks  as  large  as  six  feet  high 
and  three  feet  across  [1.83m  - 0.91m]  and  lined  with  softer  vegetation.  Nest  placement  will  normally 
be  near  the  top  of  a large  tree  on  the  edge  of  a forest  canopy.  A clutch  size  of  two  to  four  eggs  are 
commonly  laid  between  the  months  of  April  and  May  and  will  hatch  out  approximately  30  days  later. 
The  young,  referred  to  as  eyasses,(“EYE-ess-ess”)  begin  to  leave  the  nest  just  after  seven  weeks  of 
age.  The  fledglings  will  then  follow  their  parents  as  they  learn  to  fly  and  hunt  for  themselves  until 
they  can  eventually  survive  on  their  own. 

For  use  as  program  birds,  the  Raptor  Center  College  of  Veterinary  Medicine  at  the  University  of 
Minnesota  lists  the  Red-tailed  Hawk  among  a small  group  of  birds  of  prey  referred  to  as  the  “Steady 
Six”.  The  species  in  this  group  have  proven  to  be  the  easiest  to  work  with  for  educators.  The  other 
members  of  the  steady  six  are  the  American  Kestrel  (Falco  sparverius).  Great  Homed  Owl  {Bubo 
virginianus),  Northern  Saw-whet  Owl  {Aegolius  acadicus).  Eastern  Screech  Owl  {Megascops  asio), 
and  Western  Screech  Owl  {Otus  kennicottii).  To  keep  a Red-tailed  Hawk  you  must  have  a Special 
Purpose  Possession  Education  Permit,  as  required  under  the  Migratory  Bird  Treaty  Act,  as  well  as 
any  special  permits  deemed  necessary  by  the  state  of  residence.  Thanks,  Karl! 

References: 

-Species  page  from  The  Raptor  Center  at  the  University  of  Minnesota 
-Species  page  from  The  Cornell  Lab  of  Ornithology 
-The  Wikipedia  page  on  the  Red-tailed  Hawk 
-A  book  called  Raptors  in  Captivity  by  Lori  R.  Arent 


132  Animal  Keepers’ Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5 


pom 


e 


pesiuen 


I have  always  been  impressed  by  the  expert  avian  keepers  within  our  membership,  the  ones  who 
really  know  their  birds  from  crown  to  tarsus.  Fm  talking  about  the  keepers  of  indoor  aviaries  who 
can  identify  a hundred  individual  birds  and  tell  you  at  any  given  time  where  they  are  hiding  in  the 
treetops.  The  flamingo  keeper  who  can  look  out  into  the  shallows  amid  a pink  flurry  of  heads, 
wings,  and  legs  and  confidently  proclaim  she  counted  72  birds.  Are  you  kidding  me?  The  raptor 
trainer  who  gives  a bird  of  prey  the  freedom  of  flight  and  can  actually  get  the  thing  to  fly  back 
for  a measly  scrap  of  meat.  The  weekend  enthusiast  who  knows  bird  songs  better  than  a Rosetta 
Stone  graduate  knows  a foreign  language.  The  expert  ornithologist  who  unravels  the  mysteries  of 
behaviors,  vocalizations,  and  nesting  and  oversees  her  feathered  charges  from  attraction  to  courtship, 
nesting  to  eggs,  incubation  to  hatching,  rearing  to  fledging. 

This  dedicated  issue  is  for  all  of  you,  and  for  all  of  us  who  wish  we  knew  our  ornithology  half  as 
well  as  you  do.  The  collection  of  papers  is  impressive,  and  a special  thanks  goes  out  to  all  of  the 
authors,  special  editor  Diane  Olsen,  and  the  Avian  Scientific  Advisory  Group  for  helping  to  gather 
this  important  assembly  of  information. 

My  hope  is  that  we  continue  to  hear  more  from  the  bird  keepers  in  AAZK  and  this  is  just  the  start 
of  many  more  ornithological  submissions.  I also  hope  the  aquarists,  herpetologists,  and  other 
specialists  take  note  and  consider  submitting  a paper  on  their  area  of  expertise.  One  of  the  most 
important  components  of  AAZK’s  mission  is  the  sharing  of  information.  It  is  how  we  inspire 
innovation,  achieve  best  practice,  standardize  knowledge,  and  recognize  professional  achievement. 
Congratulations  to  AKF  Editor  Susan  Chan  and  the 
authors  for  achieving  just  that  and  creating  a fine 
dedicated  issue  of  the  Animal  Keepers'  Forum. 


P rom  tiiG  AKP  PJitor  . . 


2W 

Shane  Good,  President 


It  is  always  exciting  to  work  on  a special  issue  of  AKF  dedicated  to  a particulr  group  of  animals. 
One  of  the  real  “perks”  of  my  job  is  that  I learn  something  new  every  day  and  continue  to  expand 
my  animal  knowldge  base  as  members  share  with  me  their  experiences  and  expertise.  As  an  editor  I 
always  feel  humbled  when  professionals  entrust  their  manuscripts  to  me  believing  that  I will  do  my 
utmost  to  make  their  articles  turn  out  the  best  they  can  be. 

I want  to  first  thank  Diane  Olsen  of  Moody  Gardens  who  served  as  special  topic  editor  for  this 
dedicated  issue  of  AKF.  Her  support  and  advice  along  the  way  have  been  amazing.  I would  also  like 
to  thank  each  author  who  took  the  time  and  effort  to  submit  material  for  this  issue.  I believe  we  have 
been  able  to  cover  a broad  spectrum  of  topics  within  the  Avian  Husbandry  theme  and  I truly  hope  that 
the  information  herein  will  be  helpful  to  all  bird  keepers.  I also  hope  the  information  and  experiences 
detailed  in  this  issue  will  make  all  non-avian  keepers  more  aware  of  the  challenges  and  rewards  their 
colleagues  who  do  work  with  the  vast  array  of  bird  species  deal  with  on  a day-to-day  basis. 


One  of  the  most  difficult  aspects  of  putting  together  this  issue  was  to  narrow  down  the  potential  cover 
subject  photos  from  the  many  that  were  submitted.  I want  to  thank  all  those  who  took  the  time  and 
effort  to  submit  their  beautiful  color  photos  of  birds  both  in  captivity  and  in  the  wild.  I wish  we  had 
had  room  to  utilize  more  of  these  great  shots.  Your  willingness  to  share  your  photographic  talents  is 
much  appreciated. 

I hope  you  enjoy  this  Avian  Husbandry  issue  of  AKF. 


Susan  D.  Chan,  Editor 


Animal  Keepers’ Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5  133 


Learning  to  Walk  Again:  Paralysis  in  an  African  Penguin 


By  Bethany  Wlaz,  Animal  Keeper 

The  Maryland  Zoo  in  Baltimore,  Baltimore,  MD 

Bethany.wlaz@marylandzoo.org 


Background 

The  Maryland  Zoo  in  Baltimore  houses  the  largest  captive  colony  of  African  Penguins  (Spheniscus 
Demersus)  in  North  America.  The  long-standing  colony  at  Rock  Island  has  seen  its  share  of  trials 
and  tribulations  in  order  to  reach  its  success.  In  September  of  2008,  the  keepers,  management  and 
vet  staff  were  faced  with  a unique  medical  situation  that  challenged  the  typical  methods  and  ideals 
of  penguin  husbandry.  The  Maryland  Zoo  staff  used  intense  physical  therapy  to  treat  a 17-year-old 
paralyzed  African  penguin.  A dedicated  staff  willing  to  try  some  unconventional  treatments  in  avian 
husbandry  proved  anything  is  possible,  even  when  dealing  with  paralysis. 

It  was  shortly  after  the  morning  feed  that  penguin  number  825,  “Leroy,”  was  found  lying  outside 
his  nest  box  unable  to  walk  or  stand.  Keepers  approached  him  and  discovered  he  was  not  using  his 
left  leg.  Leroy  attempted  to  move,  dragging  his  foot  behind  him.  Less  than  30  minutes  prior,  he  was 
outside  swimming  in  the  moat  and  seen  walking  into  the  nest  room.  Immediately,  keepers  assumed 
it  was  a broken  bone.  After  a trip  to  the  zoo  hospital  for  x-rays  and  a complete  examination,  they 
realized  that  it  was  not  that  simple.  Unsure  what  could  have  happened,  Leroy  was  initially  treated 
with  a non-steroidal  anti-inflammatory  and  subcutaneous  fluids  and  placed  in  Rock  Island’s’  hospital 
room.  The  hospital  room  is  an  isolated  room  with  a pool  within  Rock  Island  that  allows  for  isolation 
from  the  colony,  but  is  next  to  the  nesting  room  so  that  the  birds  can  still  hear  each  other  and  maintain 
some  interaction  when  separated.  In  the  hospital  room,  he  was  given  a Plexiglass®  box  lined  with 
towels,  which  was  typically  used  for  chicks  (approximately  2x3  feet  - 0.6m  x 0.91m).  The  box 
would  provide  a safe  place  for  Leroy  away  from  the  other  birds  that  could  harm  him,  and  prevent  him 
from  attempting  to  move  thus  further  injuring  himself 

As  mysteriously  as  his  paralysis  in  his  left  leg  appeared,  the  following  day  keepers  arrived  to  find  his 
right  leg  mysteriously  lay  limp  as  well.  Zoo  staff  began  brainstorming  and  investigating  what  could 
have  caused  this  debilitation  so  suddenly  without  any  obvious  signs.  Keepers  struggled  to  find  an 
environmental  factor;  did  he  fall  getting  out  of  the  moat?  Did  he  get  in  a fight  with  another  bird?  Why 
didn’t  anyone  hear  or  see  the  fight?  Vet  staff  ran  blood  tests,  examined  radiographs  and  researched 
further  diagnostic  possibilities.  The  paralysis  only  involved  his  legs,  and  he  did  not  show  any  other 
symptoms.  He  had  good  cloacal  and  tail  function.  Further  x-rays  of  his  entire  spine  and  vertebra 
revealed  some  very  mild  changes  in  his  lower  neck  vertebra,  but  nothing  conclusive.  An  MRI  was 
arranged  at  a local  human  hospital  on  his  entire  spine  and  brain.  Possible  lesions  were  seen  in  the 
same  area  of  neck  vertebra  as  on  the  radiographs,  but  no  obvious  changes  were  revealed  to  suggest  a 
definitive  cause,  or  to  further  help  in  directing  treatment. 

Treatment/Physical  Therapy 

After  a few  days,  the  seriousness  and  complexity  that  lay  ahead  became  more  apparent.  Leroy  was 
paralyzed  and  the  chance  of  it  being  reversed  was  uncertain.  Keepers  prepared  to  make  his  living 
conditions  more  comfortable  and  medically  appropriate.  His  chick  box  was  layered  with  several 
pieces  of  two-inch  foam  and  towels.  The  towels  were  tightly  rolled  and  lined  the  entire  box  in 
order  to  encourage  an  upright  posture.  It  was  imperative  that  Leroy  stay  propped  up  for  many 
reasons.  Propping  him  up  would  help  prevent  sores  from  developing  on  his  cloacal  area  and  keel 
due  to  increased  pressure  in  those  areas.  It  also  helped  encourage  a typical  penguin  posture  thus  not 
restricting  any  breathing  since  he  would  be  in  this  position  for  extended  periods  of  time.  Perhaps 
most  importanly,  Leroy  needed  the  towels  and  padding  to  help  direct  proper  limb  alignment. 

After  consultations  with  a human  radiologist  and  a veterinary  neurologist,  vet  staff  quickly  reacted 
and  devised  a treatment  plan  to  the  best  of  their  ability.  Leroy  was  initially  treated  with  antibiotics 
and  an  antifungal  medication.  Corticosteroids  were  added  after  the  MRI  and  much  discussion  since 
they  would  increase  the  risk  of  aspergillosis  due  to  potential  immune-suppression.  However,  the 
veterinary  staff  felt  that  steroids  were  necessary,  which  would  reduce  possible  inflammation  of 
the  spinal  cord  and  therefore  decrease  compression  and  the  paralysis.  Prednisone,  an  oral  steroid. 


134  Animal  Keepers'  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5 


was  added  at  a moderately  low  dose  with  careful 
monitoring  and  weekly  blood  work. 

The  vets  also  felt  strongly  about  the  importance 
of  keeping  him  in  the  water  and  giving  him  swim 
sessions  in  the  hospital  room  pool.  Keepers  filled 
the  pool  half  way  and  put  Leroy  in  the  water.  He 
swam  as  a penguin  should  and  at  first  glance  may 
not  have  even  appeared  to  have  any  problem.  But 
it  was  evident  that  his  inability  to  walk  translated 
to  the  water  as  he  used  his  wings  alone  to  steer. 
The  first  couple  of  times  were  shaky.  It  was  an 
adjustment  for  him  to  learn  a new  sense  of  balance. 
Keepers  were  directed  to  keep  to  a strict  schedule 
of  swim  sessions  three  times  a day.  Perhaps  most 
importantly,  in  addition  to  the  medications,  and 
daily  swim  sessions,  a physical  therapy  program 
was  devised.  Keepers  were  to  perform  passive 
range  of  motion  (ROM)  exercises  in  order  to 
decrease  muscle  contraction  from  his  paralysis  and  increase  muscle  strength  in  his  lower  limbs.  Vet 
staff  hoped  if  he  regained  any  movement  in  his  legs  these  exercises  would  help  make  it  easier  for  him 
to  begin  walking  again.  The  ROM  exercises  were  perfonned  for  1 5 minutes  on  each  leg,  three  times 
a day.  The  physical  therapy  required  two  keepers,  one  to  hold  Leroy  and  the  other  to  perform  the 
motions.  The  motions  flexed  and  extended  the  tibiotarsometatarsal  (hock)  joint  and  the  stifle  (knee) 
joint.  Engaging  the  hip  joint  by  creating  a bicycle  circular  rotation  was  an  important  component  of 
the  stifle  joint  exercises. 

For  the  next  month,  Leroy  continued  to  be  in  good  spirits  and  adjusted  to  the  new  living  conditions 
and  arrangements.  He  lived  in  the  hospital  room  in  his  cushioned  box  and  was  taken  out  for  three 
to  four  hours  a day  in  order  to  receive  his  physical  therapy  and  swim  sessions.  Within  the  first  week 
of  treatments  keepers  began  to  notice  very  small  changes  in  Leroy’s  status.  It  was  soon  confirmed 
by  vet  staff  that  he  had  regained  the  first  signs  of  limited  mobility  in  his  feet.  Occasionally  Leroy 
would  extend  his  leg  in  efforts  to  get  out  of  the  pool, 
or  would  attempt  to  stand  after  pulling  himself  out 
of  the  pool.  His  strong  desire  to  simply  walk  was 
seen  through  many  attempts  of  pulling  himself  out 
of  the  pool  and  attempting  to  take  himself  back  to 
his  box.  These  actions  on  a daily  basis  forced  him 
to  push  his  body  beyond  its  current  physical  limit. 

These  little  daily  occurrences  eventually  became 
more  frequent.  It  was  difficult  for  keepers  to  detect 
progress  when  they  were  observing  him  on  a daily 
basis.  It  was  even  more  difficult  to  differentiate  an 
actual  voluntary  leg  movement  from  him  straining 
other  muscles  and  making  it  look  like  his  leg  was 
moving.  After  the  first  month  it  was  clear  to  us  that 
Leroy  was  no  longer  dealing  with  complete  paralysis, 
but  partial  paralysis,  coupled  with  his  atrophied 
muscles.  Leroy  continued  to  move  his  legs  slightly 
in  the  water  and  push  himself  to  an  upright  position  using  his  wings.  Eventually  he  was  able  to  hold 
himself  in  a standing  position  for  a couple  seconds. 

Although  keepers  felt  they  could  see  progress,  vet  staff  was  still  concerned  about  his  long-term 
prognosis.  Often  the  keepers  were  blinded  by  the  joys  of  Leroy’s  progression.  Seeing  him  attempt  to 
pull  his  leg  under  his  body,  seemed  like  the  milestone  of  all  milestones,  but  in  retrospect  Leroy  was 
still  unable  to  walk.  Staff  had  no  diagnosis  or  indication  if  the  treatments  were  actually  working.  A 
meeting  was  set  to  discuss  his  progress,  chances  of  full  recovery  and  quality  of  life.  Just  when  the 
reality  of  the  big  picture  and  slim  chance  of  full  recovery  began  to  cloud  the  optimism,  Leroy  used 


The  hospital  room  was  covered  with  mats  and 
towels  for  more  comfort.  Keepers  also  made 
Leroy  a ramp  with  Nomad®  mat  to  help  him 
exit  the  pool. 


Animal  Keepers'  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5  135 


his  legs  to  get  out  of  the  pool  and  stand  on  his  own  for 
the  first  time.  Although  the  issue  of  quality  of  life  kept 
surfacing,  Leroy  was  constantly  one  step  ahead,  almost 
as  if  he  was  trying  to  convince  staff  to  not  give  up  on 
him. 


After  the  first  month,  keepers,  management  and  vet 
staff  began  to  regain  optimism.  The  physical  therapy, 
steroids  and  swim  sessions  seemed  to  be  working  well. 
Vet  staff  decided  to  enhance  the  physical  therapy  plan 
by  changing  and  increasing  the  exercises.  A vet  who 
specializes  in  animal  physical  therapy  was  contacted  to 
devise  a more  targeted  and  intensive  program  with  the 
goal  to  increase  muscle  mass  and  function.  In  addition 
Mr.  and  Mrs.  Leroy  resting  on  the  cushions.  to  the  previous  exercises,  keepers  were  to  perform  the 

following:  a toning  massage,  reflex  stimulation  with 
resistance,  and  supported  active  ROM  exercise  twice  a day.  The  toning  massage  was  done  by  tapping 
fairly  deeply  and  firmly  on  the  leg  muscles  in  several  different  points  of  the  leg.  The  goal  of  this 
exercise  was  to  make  the  muscles  tense  up  after  being  stretched  rapidly  by  finger  pressure.  The 
reflex  stimulation  with  resistance  required  keepers  to  elicit  a reflex  while  holding  his  leg  extended  so 
that  he  could  not  respond  by  pulling  his  leg  away  in  order  to  force  the  muscles  to  work  against  the 
reflex.  The  final  exercise,  supported  active  ROM,  was  performed  by  holding  him  around  the  neck 
and  supporting  his  body  as  he  attempted  to  walk.  Over  time,  the  duration  and  walking  distance  were 
slowly  increased. 


Weeks  went  by  and  the  time-consuming  care  of  Leroy  became  routine.  Physical  therapy  became  more 
difficult  to  perfonn  as  he  gained  muscle  strength  and  with  each  motion,  there  was  more  resistance 
from  him.  Leroy  had  gained  enough  movement  and  muscle  tone  in  his  legs  and  rump  to  stand.  He  had 
also  figured  out  how  to  get  out  of  the  pool  using  both  his  legs  with  the  help  of  his  wings.  Leroy  was 
moved  out  of  his  cushioned  box  and  given  full  access  to  the  hospital  room  and  pool  at  all  times.  The 
hospital  room  was  lined  with  Nomad®  Mats  and  towels  to  cover  all  concrete.  Leroy’s  increased  time 
in  a larger  area  gave  him  many  more  opportunities  to  move  on  his  own.  He  used  his  wings  less  and 
less  and  his  feet  more  and  more.  After  two  months  of  paralysis,  treatments,  medication,  and  physical 
therapy,  Leroy  had  learned  to  walk  again.  By  no  means  was  it  a normal  penguin  walk,  but  a modified 
limp  with  hunched  posture.  It  was  still  leaps  and  bounds  from  where  Leroy  was  two  months  prior. 
Vet  staff  gave  the  ok  to  begin  decreasing  meds  and  discuss  the  possibilities  of  reintroduction  to  the 
colony. 


Problems  Encountered 

It  should  be  noted  that  in  addition  to  the  direct 
stress  and  hardships  faced  dealing  with  Leroy’s 
paralysis  alone,  many  other  secondary  problems 
occurred.  During  the  entire  treatment  process, 
Leroy  suffered  from  many  scrapes  and  lacerations 
on  his  wings.  This  was  due  to  him  supplementing 
his  walking  and  mobility  efforts  with  his  wings.  He 
was  treated  with  New  Skin®  several  times  to  stop 
and  prevent  bleeding.  Keepers  also  encountered 
many  time  management  issues.  The  Rock  Island 
team  was  in  the  height  of  their  breeding  season, 
with  12  chicks  to  care  for,  one  of  which  was  being 
hand-reared.  Approximately  half  of  each  eight- 
hour  workday  was  dedicated  to  Leroy’s  care.  With 
the  Rock  Island  staff  consisting  of  two  keepers 
per  day  who  were  caring  for  approximately  50 
penguins  plus  the  chicks,  and  round-the-clock 
care  of  Leroy,  staff  was  stretched  thin.  Physical 


Photo  shows  Leroy  using  the  comer  and  his 
wings  to  aid  in  getting  out  of  the  pool  and 
attempting  to  stand. 


136  Animal  Keepers'  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5 


therapy  was  time-consuming  and  keepers  were  unable  to  leave  him  unsupervised  in  the  pool  for  swim 
sessions.  Leroy  needed  to  be  supervised  as  he  swam,  to  prevent  him  from  banging  his  chest  against 
the  wall  in  attempts  to  get  out,  as  well  as  cutting  his  wings  on  the  steps.  Vets  feared  that  hitting  his 
chest  could  cause  more  trauma  to  his  spine  or  the  skin  covering  his  keel.  Leroy  received  minor 
lesions  on  his  feet  from  constant  scraping  in  efforts  to  move,  which  in  turn  often  became  covered  in 
fecal  matter. 

Keepers  simultaneously  dealt  with  a husbandry  management  issue.  At  the  time  of  Leroy’s  paralysis 
he  was  recommended  to  breed  by  the  Species  Survival  Plan®  and  had  a long  time  mate  of  16  years. 
This  extremely  bonded  pair  was  faced  with  a series  of  problems  when  Leroy  was  moved  in  the 
hospital  room.  Although  Leroy  was  kept  in  a small  chick  box  with  room  only  for  himself,  keepers 
immediately  assumed  that  his  mate  should  be  with  him.  Penguin  826,  “Mrs.  Leroy”  was  moved  into 
the  hospital  room  to  give  him  companionship  and  comfort  through  his  hard  time.  Unfortunately  this 
did  not  go  as  planned.  Mrs.  Leroy  was  clearly  stressed  out  in  the  hospital  room  being  away  from  her 
familiar  nest  box  and  colony  access.  Her  eating  suffered  and  there  was  an  obvious  onset  of  stereotypic 
pacing.  Within  a few  days,  it  became  clear  to  staff  that  it  was  best  for  Mrs.  Leroy’s  health  to  keep 
her  in  the  colony.  Keepers  feared  that  being  away  from  his  mate  for  so  long  could  be  detrimental  to 

Leroy’s  mental  state.  In  addition,  Mrs.  Leroy  began 
to  be  courted  by  another  penguin  in  the  colony 
during  Leroy’s  absence.  This  posed  many  problems 
and  forced  staff  to  make  decisions.  If  Mrs.  Leroy 
stayed  in  the  colony  there  was  the  risk  of  breaking 
the  16-year  bond  with  her  SSP®  recommended  mate. 
There  was  the  added  fear  that  if  Leroy  were  able 
to  someday  be  reintroduced  to  the  colony,  his  once 
long-time  companion  and  her  new  mate  might  turn 
on  him  and  further  injure  Leroy  as  he  tried  to  reunite 
with  her.  After  the  first  month,  it  was  decided  that 
it  was  best  to  put  Mr.  and  Mrs.  Leroy  together  after 
all.  Keepers  then  prepared  the  hospital  room  by 
completely  lining  the  room  with  mats,  towels  and 
cushions  to  help  pad  for  Mrs.  Leroy’s  anticipated 
pacing.  As  expected,  it  took  many  weeks  for  her  to 
become  comfortable  with  being  in  the  hospital  room 
and  regain  her  normal  eating  habits.  Mrs.  Leroy 
was  also  treated  with  an  antifungal  and  an  antibiotic  as 
a preventative  measure,  as  well  as  New  Skin®  for  minor 
scrapes  on  her  feet.  Towels  and  cushions  were  changed  and  cleaned  daily  to  pad  the  room  for  the  pair. 

Resolution  and  Current  Condition 

Approximately  three  months  had  passed  and  although  Leroy  did  not  have  a normal  or  ideal  penguin 
gait,  he  was  walking.  Vet  staff  felt  that  the  next  step  was  to  increase  his  opportunity  for  mobility.  It 
was  decided  that  the  pair  could  be  reintroduced  to  the  colony.  Keepers  discontinued  physical  therapy 
and  relied  on  Leroy’s  walking  to  continue  to  build  his  muscle  mass.  Modifications  were  made  to 
his  living  space  including:  moving  his  nest  box,  removing  a six-inch  front  from  his  nest  box,  and 
building  a ramp  into  his  box.  All  of  these  things  were  done  in  attempts  to  make  life  in  the  nest  room 
easier  for  Leroy.  The  new  location  of  his  nest  box  was  away  from  a dominant  male  penguin  that  could 
potentially  pick  on  him  and  steal  his  mate.  The  new  location  was  also  closer  to  the  door  outside  and 
provided  more  room  for  his  ramp.  The  ramp  was  a small  piece  of  fiber  grate  with  a Nomad®  Mat 
zip-tied  to  it.  Leroy’s  nest  box  was  also  temporarily  fitted  with  a Nomad®  Mat  rather  than  the  normal 
substrate  of  recycled  paper  chips.  This  was  done  to  provide  a better  grip  until  he  adjusted. 

Today  at  18,  Leroy  is  still  alive  and  thriving  in  The  Maryland  Zoo’s  colony.  He  continues  to  breed 
and  live  with  his  mate.  He  no  longer  needs  the  ramp  and  is  back  to  using  the  paper  chips  like  all  the 
other  penguins.  He  continues  to  use  a nest  box  without  a front,  decreasing  the  normal  jump  of  about 
eight  inches  to  a hop  of  about  two  inches.  He  also  receives  a brick  to  help  his  entrance  and  exit.  He 
spends  more  time  in  his  nest  box  and  lying  outside  it  then  he  did  in  the  past,  but  also  never  misses 
his  morning  swim  in  the  moat.  Leroy’s  mobility  is  still  limited,  but  leaps  and  bounds  from  where  he 


Leroy  was  forced  to  sit  backwards  on  his 
rump  and  hocks  until  he  regained  muscle. 


Animal  Keepers'  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5  137 


Acknowledgements : 

I’d  like  to  thank  all  the  veterinary  staff  involved  in  this 
case;  Ellen  Bronson,  DVM,  Allison  Wack,  DVM,  Donna 
Magid,  MD,  M.  Ed.,  and  Ariana  Siemsen,  DVM.  And 
a very  special  thank  you  to  those  who  invested  so  much 
hard  work,  and  never  gave  up  on  Leroy:  Ellen  Bronson, 
Allison  Wack,  Jennifer  Mignone,  Betty  Dipple,  and 
Barbara  Howard. 


At  left:  Leroy  today  in  the  colony  with  his  frontless 
nestbox  and  brick. 


Photos  provided  by  the  author. 


once  was.  The  only  other  negative  effect  observed 
since  his  paralysis  is  his  lack  of  molt  this  year, 
which  may  have  been  a side  effect  of  the  steroid 
administration. 


The  modified  nestbox  and  ramp  Leroy  used 
when  initially  reintroduced  into  the  colony. 


Throughout  Leroy’s  ordeal  never  once  did  his 
appetite,  overall  strength,  and  mental  will  suffer. 
He  acted  as  if  he  never  skipped  a beat  despite  losing 
mobility  in  his  legs  and  feet.  It  was  Leroy’s  obvious 
will  to  live  and  perseverance  that  inspired  The 
Maryland  Zoo  staff  to  never  give  up  on  him.  He 
showed  staff  little  by  little,  day  by  day  how  strong 
he  truly  was.  This  tragedy  could  not  have  happened 
at  a more  chaotic  time  at  Rock  Island,  yet  staff 
methodically  pushed  through  each  day.  From  the  day 
Leroy’s  paralysis  was  discovered  to  the  day  he  was 
released  into  the  colony,  staff  rode  a rollercoaster  of 
emotions.  The  rarity  of  the  injury  left  staff  clueless 
as  to  what  the  outcome  of  this  case  would  be. 
Leroy  would  not  be  alive  today  if  it  was  not  for  the 
persistent  and  courageous  vet  staff.  Although  trauma 
is  assumed  the  cause  of  paralysis,  a definitive  cause 


could  not  be  determined.  Vets  explored  every  avenue 
possible  in  order  to  find  answers  as  to  why  and  what 
could  be  done.  Their  decision  to  treat  him  with 
steroids  carried  the  significant  risk  of  exacerbating  a 
possible  aspergillus  infection,  but  it  is  believed  to  be 
a significant  part  of  what  saved  his  life.  With  close 
monitoring  he  showed  minimal  side  effects  from  the 
drugs.  It  is  not  often  physical  therapy  is  thought  of  as 
an  option  or  answer  in  avian  husbandry.  It  was  these 
unconventional  treatments,  a tremendous  amount  of 
hard  work,  and  a little  bit  of  faith  that  made  this  story 
a success.  Without  a doubt,  Leroy  holds  a special 
spot  in  the  hearts  of  everyone  involved  in  his  case 
- it  would  be  a lie  if  the  keepers  told  you  he  never 
received  special  treatment. 


Leroy’s  first  trip  outside  after  three  months 
was  successful,  but  he  still  had  significantly 
hunched  posture. 


138  Animal  Keepers'  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5 


eilifS  4\§  ODWiRSE  43 
I 4MiDM4\i  Mlltl@TOMi 


Working  in  partnership  with  zoos 
and  aquariums,  MAZUHI^  PhD 
nutritionists  develop  industry 
leading  products  specifically  for 
each  species.  From  aardvarks 
to  zebras,  we  work  hard  to  give 
your  animals  the  most  of  what 
they  need. 

For  more  information  on 
MAZURF  products-including 
nutritional  analyses  and  research 
findings-visit  www.mazuri.com 


or  call  l-800-a27-8941  for  the 
location  of  your  nearest  dealer. 
Because  when  it  comes  to  great 
nutrition,  we're  committed  to 
the  health  and  longevity  of 
every  animal. 


n 

The  liffltic  Animal  Feeding  Resource 


www.mazuri.com 


Animal  Keepers'  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5  139 


Development  of  an  Aviary  Training  Program: 

Enhancing  the  daily  management  of  Zoo  Atlanta’s  bird  collection 


By  Jennifer  Brink,  Keeper  II  and  Katie  Bagley,  Keeper  II 
Zoo  Atlanta,  Atlanta,  GA 

Managing  free-flight  birds  in  aviaries  can  be  challenging  but  very  rewarding.  Traditional  avian 
management  has  been  relatively  hands-off  with  the  primary  close  interactions  between  human  and 
bird  occurring  during  stressful  capture  and  restraint  procedures.  Many  collection  birds  are  distrustful 
of  humans  and  may  exhibit  signs  of  stress  while  in  small  areas  or  in  close  proximity  with  caretakers. 
Following  the  guidelines  of  Zoo  Atlanta’s  behavioral  management  philosophy  and  the  constantly 
evolving  zoological  standards  of  animal  husbandry,  Zoo  Atlanta  has  established  a training  program 
in  both  mixed-species  and  in  smaller  species-specific  aviaries.  Even  in  active  breeding  situations  this 
progressive  approach  is  allowing  for  closer  daily  management,  has  reduced  stress  and  has  lessened 
the  need  and  frequency  for  stressful  capture  and  restraint. 

The  training  programs  incorporate  the  use  of  a basic  stationing  behavior  through  the  use  of  movable 
shape  stations.  The  use  of  a stationing  behavior  in  daily  husbandry  allows  for  closer  observations, 
easier  shifting  and  crating,  and  increases  animal/keeper  trust,  while  mentally  stimulating  and 
enriching  the  birds.  As  we  develop  new  management  techniques  within  the  zoological  field,  we  can 
facilitate  a less  stressful  environment  that  can  benefit  staff  and  birds  alike.  This  paper  looks  at  how 
we,  as  animal  managers,  can  improve  avian  husbandry  through  training  in  variably  complex  breeding 
and  exhibit  environments. 

Introduction 

Animal  care  specialists,  regardless  of  species  expertise,  share  a common  goal;  to  give  the  best  possible 
care  to  those  animals  in  our  charge.  Captive  species  are  completely  dependent  on  our  abilities  and 
efficiencies  for  their  care.  In  the  last  20  years  zookeeping  has  changed  significantly  and  today 
more  attention  is  focused  on  behavioral  management.  This  provides  increased  mental  and  physical 
stimulation  for  the  individuals  and  has  allowed  for  improved  husbandry  practices.  As  animal  care 
specialists,  modification  and  ongoing  improvement  of  our  techniques  should  be  a priority. 

Working  with  exhibit  and  breeding  birds  can  be  demanding  in  itself  and  establishing  behavioral 
management  strategies  on  these  collections  poses  an  even  greater  challenge.  Unlike  many  other 
species,  most  exhibit  birds  have  historically  had  very  little  positive  interaction  with  their  caregivers, 
beyond  being  deliverers  of  food.  Many  of  our  established  relationships  are  negative.  At  Zoo  Atlanta 
we  are  hoping  to  change  these  relationships  through  progressive  management  using  training.  Some 
beliefs  held  about  bird  training  have  been  that:  interacting  with  the  birds  encourages  some  species 
to  bond  with  humans  rather  than  potential  mates  and  thus  negatively  affect  breeding  programs;  that 
training  is  not  worth  the  time  investment  since  many  birds  are  able  to  be  netted  and  restrained  very 
easily;  that  keeper  presence  in  the  exhibit  is  stressful  on  the  birds  and  husbandry  should  be  done 
quickly  by  servicing  the  exhibit  and  leaving.  Especially  during  capture,  our  reliance  on  nets  results 
in  mistrust  of  caretakers,  stressful  environments,  and  potential  injury  to  the  birds. 

Zoo  Atlanta’s  bird  staff  does  not  feel  this  hands-off  approach,  although  respected  and  followed  in 
appropriate  situations,  needs  to  be  the  rule.  We  are  currently  establishing  a training  program  based 
on  positive  reinforcement  for  our  exhibit  bird  collection.  This  paper  focuses  on  Zoo  Atlanta’s  Kori 
bustards  {Ardeotis  kori)  and  multi-species  walk-through  aviary.  In  conceptualizing  the  training 
program,  our  primary  focus  was  to  provide  our  birds  with  a healthier  and  more  enriching  lifestyle 
while  improving  husbandry  techniques  that  would  not  interfere  with  breeding  programs.  Since 
implementing  this  program,  we  have  discovered  many  other  benefits  including  motivating  and 
inspiring  staff  as  well  as  educating  the  public  about  bird  intelligence,  behavior,  and  physiology. 

Husbandry  Benefits 

One  of  the  target  areas  for  our  training  program  is  a free-flight,  walk-through,  African  aviary.  Fourteen 
different  species  ranging  from  Taveta  weavers  {Ploceus  castaneuceps)  to  Crested  guinea  fowl 
{Guttera  cristata)  currently  live  in  this  exhibit.  The  program  was  established  to  improve  husbandry 
in  a complex  environment  containing  many  birds.  While  some  individuals  are  comfortable  being 
seen  and  easily  observed,  others  are  more  apt  to  distance  themselves  should  a keeper  enter  their  area. 
This  makes  it  very  difficult  to  observe  all  animals  closely  and  to  fully  understand  their  behavior. 


140  Animal  Keepers'  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5 


breeding  habits,  and  physical  condition.  Since  our  program’s  inception,  training  has  provided  us  with 
much  closer  inspections  of  physical  issues.  Each  fall,  many  of  the  non-cold  tolerant  species  also  need 
to  be  brought  into  a building  to  spend  the  winter,  forcing  the  birds  into  much  closer  proximity  to  one 
another  and  also  the  keeper.  This  creates  a more  stressful  environment  for  those  already  predisposed 
to  avoid  keeper  staff  Through  training,  we  are  able  to  target  and  successfully  shift  individual  species 
into  and  between  indoor  holding  spaces  without  the  use  of  nets,  thus  decreasing  undo  stress  to  the 
individuals  targeted  and  others  residing  in  the  same  exhibit. 


Each  species  was  designated  a moveable,  colored  shape  platform  that  would  act  as  their  station. 
Ultimately,  the  species  would  recognize  their  station  and  fly  down  to  it  from  anywhere  in  the  aviary. 
The  established  behavior  can  then  be  used  to  encourage  or  train  other  behaviors,  such  as  shifting, 
crating,  and  voluntary  weighing.  This  behavior  has  allowed  easier  daily  monitoring  of  the  group. 
We  have  found  this  especially  useful  when  it  comes  to  medicating  individual  birds  in  a group  setting 
without  having  to  restrain,  separate,  or  remove  them  from  display.  It  has  lessened  the  need  for 
pulling  individuals  for  a period  of  treatment  and  largely  does  away  with  issues  that  may  result  from 

needing  to  re-introduce  an  individual 
back  to  the  group.  This  approach  may 
also  decrease  a bird’s  susceptibility 
to  diseases  where  stress  can  be  a 
contributory  factor. 

As  with  our  mixed-species  aviary, 
working  with  birds  in  single  species 
exhibits  has  also  proven  beneficial. 
Through  our  program,  we  have  been 
able  to  get  weights  on  the  Koris  as 
often  as  needed,  as  well  as  successfully 
shifting  them  into  holding  as  needed 
without  having  to  herd  them.  In 
congruence  with  a goal  of  the  Kori 
SSP®,  we  are  working  to  see  a valuable 
correlation  with  behavior  and  seasonal 
Movable,  color-coded  shape  stations  weight  fluctuations  that  would  not  be 

feasible  without  our  training  program. 
Since  beginning  this  program  the  birds 
have  shown  significant  behavioral  changes  in  the  presence  of  keeper  staff  and  are  visibly  more 
relaxed. 


Motivating  Birds 

When  beginning  any  training  program  it  is  essential  to  determine  the  animals’  motivation  to  participate. 
Birds  that  are  pursued  and  captured  using  a net  most  likely  equate  the  situation  to  being  pursued  by 
a predator.  Uncomfortable  handling  by  caretakers  typically  follows  the  initial  capture.  As  exhibit 
birds  have  generally  all  had  the  experience  of  being  physically  restrained  and/or  caught  with  a net 
multiple  times,  they  likely  associate  fear  and  discomfort  with  their  caretakers.  Several  motivational 
aspects,  including  bird/caretaker  relationships,  inter-specific  and  intra- specific  relationships,  timing 
of  training  sessions,  environment,  and  food  availability,  as  well  as  its  use  as  a reinforcer,  need  to  be 
addressed  when  beginning  a training  program. 

Bird/caretaker  relationships:  The  relationship  between  bird  and  keeper  is  extremely  important. 
Establishing  this  relationship  is  underlined  by  the  understanding  that  everything  done  in  caring  for 
these  animals  is  in  some  way  training.  Creating  a successful  program  relies  on  noticing  your  behavior 
and  actions  along  with  the  birds’  responses  and  capitalizing  on  that  knowledge.  Through  the  use  of 
enrichment  items  and  training  opportunities  throughout  the  day,  the  amount  of  negative  interactions 
between  keepers  and  birds  can  be  decreased  thus  creating  a more  trusting  and  calmer  environment. 

Inter-specific  and  intra-specific  relationships:  Determining  dominant  or  aggressive  animals  is 
important.  Focusing  training  on  dominant  individuals  first  avoids  inter-  and  intra-specific  aggression. 
By  doing  so,  less  dominant  birds  also  become  more  motivated  to  participate  after  watching  the  others 
getting  rewarded.  Throughout  the  training  process  we  continue  to  observe  hierarchal  changes  that  are 
associated  with  breeding  status,  numbers  of  individuals  in  a species,  and  seasonal  enclosure  changes. 
This  aspect  is  not  seen  as  a training  deterrent,  but  allows  us  to  increase  our  understanding  of  the  social 


Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5  141 


Gold-breasted  and  Superb  Starlings  stationing. 


dynamics,  as  some  behaviors  may  not 
have  been  as  noticeable  prior  to  training. 
These  shifts  in  dominance  are  a constant 
challenge  and  training  approaches  need 
to  be  modified  with  the  societal  changes 
within  the  exhibit/enclosure. 

Timing  of  training  sessions  and  food:  Timing 
of  training  sessions  is  extremely  important 
when  establishing  a program.  Mtially, 
sessions  were  held  first  thing  in  the  morning 
when  the  birds  were  relaxed  and  highly  food- 
motivated.  As  weight  management  is  not 
used  as  a training  tool,  favored  food  items  are 
withheld  and  used  for  the  training  sessions. 
Eventually  we  were  able  to  estabhsh  a late 
morning  training  presentation  and  keeper 
talk.  By  doing  so,  we  were  able  to  capitalize 
on  keeper  time  by  successfully  combining 
husbandry  needs  with  the  demands  of  zoo 
promotional  initiatives. 


Environment:  During  the  course  of  our  training  we  have  found  that  the  birds’  motivation  to  train  is 
sometimes  challenged  by  the  exhibit  and  environment  in  which  the  birds  live.  Continually  evaluating 
bird  behavior  and  their  exhibits  plays  an  important  role  in  training  programs.  As  many  bird  exhibits 
are  not  planned  and  built  with  training  programs  in  mind,  minor  modifications  are  necessary  to 
encourage  a more  inviting  environment  for  the  birds.  Physical  and  behavioral  barriers  resulting  from 
their  environment  affects  the  willingness  of  the  birds  to  train.  In  establishing  a training  program, 
it  forces  keepers  and  curators  to  evaluate  the  animals’  environment.  Training  can  only  occur  if  the 
trainee  is  calm,  comfortable,  and  motivated.  In  proceeding  with  our  training  programs  we  found  that 
adjustments  needed  to  be  made  to  exhibits  and  husbandry  to  create  a more  comfortable  environment. 
Simple  steps  made  in  the  Kori  husbandry  included  removing  visual  barriers  from  their  indoor  holding 
area  and  providing  a heat  pad  on  exhibit  to  lessen  the  need  to  lock  them  inside  during  inclement 
weather.  These  adjustments,  along  with  others,  opened  up  a greater  training  window  and  also 
prompted  keepers  to  research  more  about  the  species.  This  research  inspired  a change  in  husbandry 
and  encouraged  a more  inviting  environment  in  which  the  birds  can  live  and  thrive. 


Many  birds  are  highly  sensitive  to  changes  in  their  environment.  When  observing  the  birds  in  the 
walk-through  African  exhibit  and  in  their  winter  holding,  we  observe  many  approaches  made  by 
the  birds  to  their  station  and  crate:  direct  flights,  perch-to-perch,  hovering,  and  walking.  Moveable 
stations  are  advantageous  in  these  situations 
because  we  are  able  to  position  the  station 
based  on  the  approaching  behaviors  of  each 
species.  For  example,  the  Superb  starlings 
(Spreo  superbus),  fly  directly  to  their  station 
while  in  the  aviary  but  hesitantly  hover  over 
the  station  inside  winter  holding.  Positioning 
the  station  near  staggered  perches  increases 
their  comfort  and  motivation  level.  This 
method  also  helps  to  decrease  interference 
from  species  that  approach  the  stations  in 
other  ways.  It  also  taught  the  keepers  much 
more  about  these  specific  birds  allowing 
us  to  improve  exhibit  set-ups  in  regard  to 
perching.  We  were  then  also  able  to  provide 
more  accessible  nesting  areas,  and  better 
feeding  opportunities. 


Breeding 

An  active  and  controlled  breeding  program  is 
generally  essential  to  support  a zoo’s  mission. 


Cape  Teal  on  station. 


142  Animal  Keepers'  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5 


and  is  consequently  of  high  importance.  Some  of  us  in  the  bird  community  have  had  concerns  about 
the  possible  negative  effects  of  training  on  breeding  birds.  Will  it  create  bird/human  bonds  that 
could  interfere  with  pairing?  Will  it  damage  existing  pair  bonds?  Is  it  safe  to  train  birds  while  they 
are  actually  breeding?  Training  can,  in  fact,  do  the  opposite  of  interfering.  When  done  properly  it 
can  benefit  and  encourage  without  impacting  pair  bonds.  Through  training,  the  birds  in  our  care  are 
developing  a mutual  trust  with  us.  This  trust  appears  to  lessen  the  amount  of  stress  they  feel  in  our 
presence  thereby  calming  their  interactions  with  their  mate  and  allowing  for  more  accurate  physical 
and  behavioral  observations  by  keepers. 

Prior  to  implementing  our  training  program  we  established  protocols  in  an  attempt  to  ensure  that 
breeding  and  pair  bonds  were  not  impacted.  To  keep  the  pair  bond  between  the  birds  solidified,  hand 
feeding  is  not  permitted.  Reward  items  are  generally  tossed  or  dropped  to  the  bird.  During  rearing 
season,  birds  are  left  with  a constant  supply  of  all  food  to  ensure  pairs  can  feed  their  growing  chicks. 
Since  establishing  our  training,  our  breeding  program  has  continued  to  be  successful.  During  nesting 
and  rearing  season,  birds  continue  to  participate  in  their  training  in  a modified  form.  Sessions  were 
restricted  to  the  mornings,  and  adults  would  take  their  food  reward  to  their  respective  nests  to  feed 
their  chicks  and/or  mate.  Multiple  chicks  from  several  species  have  been  reared  since  the  program 
began  including  Superb  starlings.  White-headed  buffalo  weavers  {Dinemellia  dinemelli),  and 
Violaceous  turacos  {Musophaga 
violacea). 

A male  Buffalo  weaver  with  a 
history  of  aggressive  behavior  had 
been  trained  to  enter  a shift  cage 
prior  to  being  introduced  to  his 
current  mate.  Once  introduced 
to  each  other  they  soon  nested. 

When  the  pair  began  rearing  a 
chick,  he  was  no  longer  asked  to 
perform  this  behavior.  After  the 
chick  fledged,  keepers  were  able 
to  quickly  re-establish  his  shifting. 

Currently  he,  his  mate,  and  his 
0.1  offspring  are  all  involved  in 
the  aviary  station  training  with 
no  change  in  behavior  or  pair 
bonding  shown. 

In  addition  to  observing  calmer 
behavior  from  pairs,  we  have  also  noticed  added  benefits  with  their  chicks.  Offspring  that  have  been 
reared  with  the  training  program  in  place  have  been  taught  through  their  parents  and  are  markedly 
calmer.  Although  uncertain  about  how  training  impacts  breeding  programs  of  all  avian  species, 
some  of  our  most  successful  breeding  pairs  this  year  were  involved  in  training.  All  breeding  pairs 
involved  in  Zoo  Atlanta’s  program  continue  behaving  normally  with  those  in  their  social  group  and 
are  successful  breeders.  When  used  judiciously  we  believe  that  we  should  be  able  to  avoid  negative 
outcomes  to  our  training  and  still  produce  excellent,  calm  exhibit/breeding  birds. 

Staff  Impact 

As  with  any  facility.  Zoo  Atlanta  keepers  deal  with  a multitude  of  species  and  exhibits  and  time 
is  extremely  valuable.  Finding  and  justifying  the  time  to  train  can  be  challenging.  As  such, 
establishing  priorities  within  a departmental  training  program  is  extremely  important.  Larger  multi- 
species exhibits  and  less  easily  handled  species  have  been  given  priority  over  single-species  aviaries. 
While  time  may  be  limited,  training  is  not  impossible.  Our  training  program  began  with  just  a few 
minutes  each  day  trying  to  capitalize  on  the  bird  and  trainer’s  motivation.  In  the  long  run,  these 
few  moments  out  of  the  day  can  have  very  important  staffing  benefits.  Eliminating  or  decreasing 
the  number  of  personnel  needed  to  capture  a bird  can  increase  productivity  and  efficiency  of  the 
staff  Since  implementing  our  program,  there  have  been  multiple  occasions  on  which  a single  keeper 
accomplished  what  had  previously  taken  multiple  keepers. 

While  this  type  of  training  shares  similarities  with  other  training  programs,  it  has  its  own  set  of 
challenges  providing  an  opportunity  to  inspire  and  motivate  keepers  to  continually  search  for  better 


Gold-breasted  Starling  looking  into  crate 


Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5  143 


ways  to  care  for  and  understand  species  that  they  train.  As  with  many  other  professions  and  careers, 
employees  who  are  not  challenged  or  inspired  can  become  dissatisfied  and  move  towards  other  jobs 
and  companies.  High  quality  staff  members  are  those  who  contemplate  and  question  techniques, 
and  modify  them  to  better  the  quality  of  husbandry  they  provide.  These  behavioral  management 
programs  require  keepers  to  research,  learn,  and  understand  psychological  and  physical  aspects  of  the 
birds  while  teaching  valuable  problem-solving  capabilities.  Although  this  is  not  the  primary  focus  of  J 
the  program,  when  ehallenged  and  inspired  and  rewarded  many  keepers  do  put  forth  more  effort  into  ii 
their  work.  Many  staff  love  training  and  the  benefits  are  not  confined  to  the  birds.  Training  is  a huge 
motivator  for  many  people.  It  can  be  enrichment  to  humans  and  animals  alike.  ii 

Public  Programming  I 

At  Zoo  Atlanta,  as  with  many  other  institutions,  there  has  become  a growing  demand  on  keepers  for  ji 

improving  and  increasing  guest  interaction.  It  is  our  responsibility  as  animal  keepers  and  managers  j 

to  provide  and  promote  high-quality  animal  husbandry  at  our  institutions.  It  is  our  responsibility  as  || 

zoo  employees  to  not  only  educate  guests  and  support  conservation,  but  to  promote  our  institutions.  | 

More  attention  is  being  focused  on  guest  interaction,  entertainment  value,  and  stay  time  at  exhibits. 

The  desire  to  follow  Zoo  Atlanta’s  initiatives,  while  not  detracting  from  keeper  work  time,  has  resulted 
in  keeper  talks  revolving  around  our  bird  training  program.  Both  Kori  and  aviary  training  sessions  ; 
are  advertised  events  for  guests.  As  animal  caregivers  it  is  foremost  in  our  minds  to  be  thinking  about 
husbandry,  breeding,  conservation,  and  other  messages  we  want  to  share  with  our  guests.  Many  of  our 
guests  receive  these  messages  best  when  they  are  being  “entertained”  by  keeper/animal  interaction  ; 
that  draws  the  animal  into  a more  visible  area.  Most  keepers  involved  in  training  are  passionate  about 
it  and  are  therefore  more  likely  to  provide  interesting  and  infonnative  talks  to  the  public.  While 
fornial  surveys  have  not  been  done,  it  has  been  found  that  both  of  these  programs  have  increased  the  ! 

guest  stay  time  at  the  respective  exhibits.  Many  docents  have  become  involved  as  well;  willing  to 
stay  and  speak  to  the  public  about  the  importance  of  training  in  such  complex  environments.  Not 
only  have  we  seen  guests  become  interested  in  the  birds  themselves,  but  it  also  prompts  discussions 
about  animal  care  in  captivity  and  advances  made  in  animal  husbandry.  During  breeding  season  it 
has  prompted  discussions  about  SSP®s,  animal 
management  in  AZA  institutions,  and  the 
improving  husbandry  standards.  Our  guests 
leave  these  talks  and  training  demonstrations 
with  an  increased  appreciation  for  birds  of  all 
kinds,  their  intelligence,  social  structure,  and 
behavior;  as  well  as  a better  understanding  of 
zoos  and  their  mission. 

Conclusion 

As  zoological  institutions  are  eontinuously 
changing  and  evolving,  so  should  our  behavioral 
and  husbandry  management  techniques. 

Behavioral  management  programs  have 
become  a strong  focus  within  AZA  facilities 
and  are  considered  a basic  part  of  most  mammal 
husbandry.  Bird  management  is  lagging  behind 
in  this  field.  Although  it  may  be  possible  to 
maintain  adequate  avian  husbandry  practices 
without  the  implementation  of  behavioral 
management  programs,  the  benefits  of  it  greatly 
outweigh  the  negatives.  With  the  establishment 
of  AZA’s  Standardized  Guidelines  for  Animal 
Care  it  is  timely  for  avian  specialists  to  be 

looking  at  all  aspects  of  our  husbandry  programs  and  detemiining  better  and  more  in-depth  ways  of 
managing  and  understanding  our  animals.  Training  is  an  important  step  toward  that  goal.  Training 
programs  benefit  all  those  involved  whether  it  is  the  birds  themselves,  keepers,  management,  veterinary 
staff,  or  even  our  guests.  It  is  our  responsibility  as  bird  keepers  and  managers  to  not  only  maintain 
existing  care,  but  to  strive  to  set  higher  standards  of  animal  care. 

Editors  Note:  The  authors  presented  this  paper  at  lAATE  2007  and  AZA  Regional  in  Denver  2007. 

Photos  provided  by  the  author. 


Gold-breasted  Starling  stationing. 


144  Animal  Keepers’ Eorum,Vol.  37  No.  4/5 


Hand-Rearing  Caribbean  Flamingos  (Phoenicoptuerus  rubber) 
and  Chilean  Flamingo  (Phoenicopterus  chilensis)  Chicks 
at  the  Birmingham  Zoo 

By  Paul  Smith,  Bird  Keeper;  Jejf  Fribble,  Zoological  Manager  of  Birds; 

Cindy  Finger,  Curator  of  Birds,  Birmingham  Zoo,  Birmingham,  AL 


Introduction 

During  the  summers  of  2007  and  2009  Birmingham  Zoo  received  eggs  from  the  collection  flock 
of  Chilean  flamingos  {Phoenicopterus  chilensis)  at  Zoo  Atlanta  in  Atlanta,  GA.  Thirty  Chilean 
Flamingos  were  hatched  and  raised  with  a 76%  survival  rate.  In  2007  we  also  received  Caribbean 
Flamingo  {Phoenicopterus  ruber)  eggs  from  the  flock  that  resides  at  the  Hialeah  Race  Track  in  Florida. 
Fourteen  Caribbean  Flamingos  were  hatched  with  a 93%  survival  rate.  All  of  these  flamingos  were 
fed  a diet  that  was  created  by  Ellen  Dierenfeld,  Ph.D.  and  the  Bronx  Zoo. 

Transportation 

Zoo  staff  transported  the  eggs  to  the  Birmingham  Zoo.  The  Chilean  eggs  were  transported  by  car 
from  Atlanta  to  Birmingham,  approximately  a two-hour  trip.  The  Caribbean  eggs  were  transported 
by  car  also  but  the  trip  was  about  13  hours.  The  flamingo  eggs  were  transported  in  an  ice  chest 
equipped  with  hot  water  bottles  to  maintain  temperature.  A digital  thermometer  with  a long  lead 
was  placed  at  the  level  of  the  eggs  with  the  display  module  outside.  Inside  the  cooler  we  placed  a 
layer  of  egg  crate  foam  as  a cushion.  We  used  smaller  pieces  of  cut  foam  to  help  provide  protection 
between  the  individual  eggs.  Another  layer  of  foam  went  on  top  of  the  eggs  for  further  padding  and 
as  insulation. 

The  container  was  maintained  at  a stable  temperature  of  around  98.5°F  [36.9°C]  for  the  duration  of 
the  trip  from  Atlanta  to  Birmingham.  Upon  arrival  at  the  Birmingham  Zoo  the  eggs  were  transferred 
into  an  incubator. 

Incubation 

Flamingos  typically  incubate  for  approximately  28-30  days.  The  incubation  temperature  for  Chilean 
flamingos  is  98.49°F  [36.94°C]  with  60%  humidity.  The  incubation  temperature  for  Caribbean 
Flamingos  is  just  slightly  higher  at  98.6°F  [37.2°C],  with  a humidity  of  60%. 

The  chicks  are  very  vocal  in  the  egg  right  before  hatching.  Calling  back  to  the  chick  in  the  egg 
may  help  stimulate  hatching.  Once  the  chicks  entered  the  air  cell,  their  egg  was  moved  from  the 
incubator  into  a hatcher.  The  chicks  typically  hatched  within  24-48  hours  after  entering  the  egg  air 
cell.  Hatcher  temperature  was  kept  slightly  lower  than  the  incubator  temperature,  approximately 
98°F  [36.67°C]  and  80%  humidity. 

Diet 

In  the  past  when  hand-rearing  flamingos  Birmingham  Zoo  has  used  a diet  formula  consisting  of 
cooked  egg  yolk,  cereal  and  fish/krill  blended  together.  That  diet  was  adjusted  as  the  chicks  matured 
into  three  variations  gradually  reducing,  and  then  eliminating,  the  cereal  and  egg  yolk  portion. 
However  we  have  had  success  in  recent  years  with  a much  simpler  formula,  based  on  that  used  by 
the  Bronx  Zoo: 


Whole  hardboiled  egg  (1  whole  egg) 

50g 

Hardboiled  egg  yolk  (4  yolks) 

lOOg 

Water 

150ml 

Calcium  Carbonate  (high  purity) 

2g 

Vitamin  E supplement 

20-25  lU 

Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5  145 


This  diet  provides  58%  fat,  32%  protein,  1.3%  calcium,  0.8%  phosphorus  (DM  basis),  at  23%  solids. 
Calculated  energy  content  of  this  revised  formula  is  substantially  higher  than  earlier  formulas  due 
to  the  increased  fat  level,  and  should  support  normal  growth  and  development.  Due  to  the  fact  that 
egg  is  the  exclusive  protein  source,  no  limiting  amino  acids  are  detected  and  this  protein  should  be  of 
highest  quality  and  balance  for  growing  chicks  (Dierenfeld,  2005). 

This  diet  tends  to  harden  in  the  chick’s  crop  if  the  formula  is  too  thick  and/or  the  chicks  are  dehydrated. 
We  increased  the  amount  of  water  in  the  diet  as  necessary.  We  added  an  extra  lOcc  to  50cc  of  water 
(depending  on  volume  of  the  batch)  per  batch  for  the  first  couple  of  weeks. 

Do  not  feed  a chick  that  has  a food  lump  in  its  crop.  Gently  massage  the  crop  to  break  up  the  lump 
as  much  as  possible.  Give  water  instead  of  diet  at  scheduled  feeding,  more  often  if  necessary.  Water 
should  be  administered  between  feedings  if  a chick  has  been  having  continued  problems  with  food 
lumps  as  this  could  be  a sign  of  dehydration. 

Feeding 

The  chicks  were  fed  using  syringes  with  catheter  tips  with  flexible  catheters  attached.  For  these  early 
feedings,  a 1 Occ  syringe  was  used,  but  it  quickly  became  necessary  to  move  up  to  a 60cc  syringe. 
This  larger  size  was  used  through  the  weaning  process.  The  catheters  were  cut  to  varying  lengths 
depending  on  the  bird’s  size.  The  cut  ends  of  the  catheters  were  gently  heated  to  round  the  sharp 
edges  before  its  initial  use. 

Feedings  initially  occurred  roughly  every  two  hours  starting  at  0800hrs  and  ending  at  ITOOhrs.  The 
feedings  started  as  soon  as  the  chicks  metabolized  the  internal  yolk  sac.  This  took  between  12  and 
24  hours  after  hatching,  although  fluids  were  given  before  this  when  dehydration  was  a concern. 
The  formula  was  heated  to  around  36°C  (96.8°F).  The  chicks  will  refuse  food  if  it  is  too  cold  and 
there  is  a the  risk  of  burning  the  crop  if  it  is  too  hot.  When  feeding,  the  crop  was  filled  until  it  was 
almost  completely  full.  Avoid  filling  the  crop  completely  as  this  increases  the  risk  of  aspirating  into 
the  lungs.  This  was  mainly  a concern  during  the  first  couple  of  days  since  a swallowing  response  in 
many  of  the  chicks  is  not  yet  developed.  Feeding  response  was  usually  gained  by  touching  the  side 
of  the  bill  with  the  feeding  tube.  The  chick  opened  its  beak  and  the  tube  was  placed  down  the  throat 
passed  the  tracheal  opening.  There  was  a stronger  swallowing  response  when  the  chick  was  standing 
with  its  neck  held  straight.  This  was  accomplished  by  holding  the  thumb  and  index  finger  around 
the  bill  and  once  you  had  a feeding  response  then  gently  tilting  the  chick’s  head.  In  our  experience 
the  Caribbean  flamingos  became  more  comfortable  with  this  feeding  method  than  the  Chileans.  As 
a general  rule,  they  were  much  more  prone  to  struggling  and  trying  to  avoid  the  tube.  This  was  not 
a significant  problem  as  the  chicks  still  exhibited  signs  of  hunger  and  early  feeding  responses.  It 
merely  required  a slightly  firmer  hand  when  holding  the  Chilean  chick’s  head. 

Feeding  frequency  was  reduced  to  four  times  a day  at  around  seven  days  of  age.  At  14  days  old  we 
went  to  three  feedings  a day.  At  this  point  shallow  pans  of  water  with  flamingo  pellets  were  provided 
and  most  of  the  chicks  began  learning  to  filter  feed  within  three  or  four  days.  It  is  important  to  feed 
a diet  rich  in  calcium  for  at  least  the  first  year.  At  the  Birmingham  Zoo  we  feed  Mazuri®  Flamingo 
Breeder  year-round. 

As  long  as  weight  development  remained  on  track  (approximately  a 1 0%  gain  per  day)  feedings  were 
dropped  to  twice  daily  at  28  days.  The  average  amount  of  formula  per  feeding  at  this  stage  was  60cc. 
Chicks  were  reliably  filter  feeding  at  this  age  so  the  keepers  were  tube  feeding  on  a more  supplemen- 
tal basis.  At  just  over  a month  old  most  of  the  chicks  were  reduced  to  a single  feeding  a day,  and  by 
42  days  of  age  all  tube  feeding  was  stopped.  There  were  a couple  of  chicks  that  required  one  or  two 
additional  feeds  in  the  days  that  followed  when  weight  gain  began  to  fall  behind,  but  all  chicks  were 
fully  self-feeding  by  this  time. 


146  Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5 


Days  Old 

Number  of  Feedings 

Dours  Beivveen  | 

Feedings 

1-7 

5 

2 

8-14 

4 

2.5 

15-28 

3 

4 

29-35 

2 

AM  and  PM 

35-40 

1 1 Mid  Day 

Housing 

For  brooder  boxes  we  used  plastic  boxes  measuring  18”  wide  x 24”  long  x 14”  deep  [45.7cm  x 
60.9cm  X 35.6cm]  with  the  soft,  rubberized  drawer  liner  (Grip-It®  Shelf  & Drawer  Liner)  cut  to  size 
to  line  the  bottom.  This  lining  material  is  easy  to  clean,  soft  on  the  chicks  feet  and  provides  enough 
grip  to  allow  the  chicks  to  move  about  comfortably.  Additionally,  250-watt  heat  lamps  were  hung 
above  the  boxes  at  varying  heights  to  allow  keepers  to  control  the  brooder  temperature,  kept  at 
around  98.06°F  [36.7°C]  for  the  first  few  days  and  then  gradually  reduced  to  93.02°F  [33.9°C]  by 
Day  7.  By  Day  14  the  temperatures  were  held  near  89.96°F  [32.2°C]  and  reduced  84.92°F  [29.4°C] 
by  the  Day  22. 

We  found  that  aggression  became  an  issue  if  more  than  four  chicks  were  put  in  a brooder  box 
together.  Aggression  was  particularly  a problem  between  chicks  of  significantly  different  age/size. 
My  anecdotal  observation  was  that  the  females  were  more  aggressive  than  the  males,  but  perhaps  this 
was  unintentional  chauvinism  on  my  part.  As  the  chicks  grew  and  became  more  aggressive,  certain 
problem  chicks  were  housed  separately. 

Once  the  chicks  were  too  large  to  be  housed  in  brooder  boxes,  approximately  four  weeks  of  age,  they 
were  moved  into  our  flamingo  holding  facility  in  an  area  sectioned  off  from  our  resident  flock.  This 
provided  the  chicks  not  only  with  more  space  to  move  and  larger  basins  for  feeding  and  bathing,  but 
also  allowed  them  to  be  near  and  interact  vocally  with  our  adult  birds.  Both  groups  were  allowed 
to  observe  each  other  for  a week  before  the  chicks  were  integrated  with  the  adults  during  the  days. 
For  a couple  of  weeks  the  chicks  were  placed  back  into  the  original  holding  area  separated  from  the 
adults  overnight.  After  this  two-week  introduction  period  the  chicks  were  left  permanently  in  with 
the  adults  day  and  night. 

Husbandry 

Each  chick  was  fitted  with  a number  five  size  plastic  band  for  identification.  Since  the  eight  chicks 
that  survived  hatched  several  days  apart  from  each  other  it  was  easy  to  tell  them  apart  for  a little 
while.  The  bands  were  eventually  necessary  at  around  a week  old.  These  bands  were  increased 
in  size  two  more  times  during  the  chick’s  development  while  at  our  facility.  Pinioning  was  done 
between  seven  and  ten  days  of  age.  At  the  Birmingham  Zoo  we  use  the  long  pinioning  technique  on 
all  of  our  flamingos. 

Starting  at  seven  days  of  age,  the  chicks  were  taken  out  into  an  exercise  yard  for  two,  half-hour 
intervals  daily.  The  exercise  yard  was  a section  of  lawn  set  apart  from  other  animal  exhibits  but 
viewable  by  the  public  visitors  to  the  zoo.  In  the  past,  when  hand-rearing  the  Caribbean  flamingo 
chicks  we  had  a fenced  off  area  used  as  an  exercise  yard.  We  found  that  the  Chileans  were  less  likely 
to  stray  from  the  keeper  and  tended  to  stay  closer  to  each  other,  so  we  did  not  fence  their  exercise 
area. 

The  chicks  had  a tendency  to  eat  anything  that  they  could  pick  up,  so  the  area  was  kept  as  free  of 
leaf  litter  and  detritus  as  possible.  If  a chick  ingested  too  much  of  this  material  it  could  have  caused 
crop  or  intestinal  blockage.  During  these  exercise  sessions  the  birds  were  supervised  by  at  least  one 
keeper,  and  even  with  a relatively  clean  yard  it  was  often  necessary  to  remove  objects  from  their 
mouths  before  they  could  swallow  them. 

Daily  bathing  was  needed  to  keep  the  birds  clean,  so  shallow  tubs  of  water  were  provided  during 
exercise  sessions.  Because  chicks  were  slow  to  take  to  bathing  themselves,  they  were  frequently 
bathed  by  the  keepers. 


Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5  147 


Growth  and  Development 

Once  the  chicks  began  to  gain  weight,  they  did  so  at  approximately  1 0%  of  their  body  weight  per  day. 
(Figure  1 and  Figure  2)  The  first  couple  of  days  it  is  normal  for  them  to  lose  weight.  The  chicks  were 
weighed  daily  to  monitor  growth  and  development. 

We  did  have  some  medical  issues  arise  in  the  Chilean  flamingos  that  we  did  not  see  in  the  American 
Flamingos.  Within  the  first  couple  of  days  after  hatching  we  had  nine  chicks  develop  symptoms  of 
a curvature  in  the  neck  where  they  could  not  hold  their  head  upright.  The  head  was  positioned  in  a 
way  that  it  was  looped  around  and  they  were  looking  at  the  ceiling.  It  was  determined  that  this  was  a 
bacterial  infection  and  a two-week  treatment  of  Nystatin  and  Baytril  was  started  with  much  success. 

Chiiiean  Flamingo  Growth  Curve 


Days  Old 

Ifigure  1:  Average  Growth  for  21  Chilean  Flamingos 


Conclusion 

Flamingos  can  be  time- 
consuming  to  rear  but  they 
are  not  difficult.  It  is  always 
best  to  watch  your  animals 
and  provide  them  with  what 
they  require.  Amounts  that  are 
fed  and  brooder  temperatures 
can  change  depending  on  the 
individual  bird. 


American  Flamingo  Growth  Chart 


Days  Old 

Figure  2:  Average  Growth  for  13  Amercian  Flamingos 


Chilean  Flamingo  Chick  at  14  days 

of  age.  {Photo  by  Alan  Yester) 


References 

Ellen  S.  Dierenfeld,  Ph.D.,  2005.  Personnel 
Communication  through  Tim  Snyder. 

Pribble,  Jeff,  C.  Pinger,  and  T.  Snyder.  2003.  “Hand- 
Rearing  Caribbean  Flamingo  [Phoenicopterus  ruber 
ruber]  Chicks  at  the  Birmingham  Zoo.”  AAZK 
Conference  Proceedings. 


148  Animal  Keepers'  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5 


Breeding  Cinereous  Vultures  (Aegypius  monachus) 
at  The  Milwaukee  County  Zoo 


By  Dawn  Wicker,  Area  Supervisor,  Winter  Quarters 
Milwaukee  County  Zoo,  Milwaukee,  WI 

Cinereous  Vultures,  or  Eurasian  Black  Vultures,  {Aegypius  monachus)  are  large  vultures  native  to 
parts  of  Europe,  Northern  Africa,  the  Middle  East,  and  Asia.  It  is  estimated  that  there  are  less  than 
10,000  pairs  in  the  wild;  thus  they  are  considered  '‘Near  Threatened”.  One  main  threat  is  mortality 

at  the  hands  of  humans.  They  are  sometimes 
deliberately  killed,  and  they  also  consume 
poisoned  bait  intended  for  other  predators. 
The  decline  of  available  food  is  another  major 
problem. 

In  the  North  American  collection,  there  are 
slightly  less  than  50  of  these  birds  held  in 
23  institutions.  The  TAG  recommended 
population  is  75  individuals.  Unfortunately, 
despite  the  number  of  recommended  pairs, 
there  has  been  a low  production  of  viable 
offspring.  Broken  eggs,  infertile  eggs,  chick 
death  in  the  shell,  and  death  early  after  hatch 
are  all  problems. 

The  Milwaukee  County  Zoo  has  had  Cinereous 
vultures  since  at  least  1970  when  three  wild-caught  birds  were  acquired.  Unfortunately,  all  three 
individuals  were  males.  In  the  mid-eighties,  under  the  direction  of  then-Curator  of  Birds  Ed  Diebold, 
a breeding  program  was  developed.  Although  there  were  some  early  struggles,  including  one 
incorrectly  sexed  bird,  a chick  was  hatched  in  1988.  This  chick  was  hand-reared,  and  was  sent  to  the 
Lincoln  Park  Zoo  in  Chicago  in  1989. 

Here  at  the  Milwaukee  County  Zoo,  Cinereous  vultures  are  exhibited  in  a mixed-species  exhibit  with 
Impala  (Aepyceros  melampus).  Gazelles  {Gazella  dama),  and  Sulcata  Tortoises  {Geochelone  sulcata) 
from  approximately  mid-April  through  October,  depending  on  the  weather.  In  the  winter,  they  are 
set  up  off-exhibit  in  pairs  for  breeding.  Over  the  years  this  has  been  done  in  a variety  of  places.  In 
the  late  80’s  and  early  90’s,  Cinereous  vultures  were  usually  wintered  outdoors.  They  had  a fairly 
good-sized  yard  with  a lean-to  shelter  inside  it.  Because  the  food  and  water  would  freeze,  the  birds 
were  tended  to  twice  daily. 

By  the  mid-1990s,  the  vultures  were  generally  housed  for  the  winter  in  “Bird  Winter  Quarters”,  an 
indoor  area  connected  to  the  Zoo’s  then-Hospital.  One  female  had  a joint  problem  in  one  leg,  and  it 
was  decided  that  it  might  be  too  cold  for  her  outside.  Large  cyclone  fence  enclosures  with  concrete 
floors  were  set  up  with  breeding  platforms,  approximately  4’x4’  [1.2m  x 1.2m]  and  about  2’  high 
[.60m].  The  birds  were  in  relative  seclusion;  a keeper  came  once  a day  to  feed  and  clean,  and  then 
the  birds  were  left  in  peace. 

Starting  in  2004,  the  Cinereous  vultures  have  been  kept  in  the  same  underground  bam  as  the  animals 
with  which  they  share  the  exhibit  yard,  called  “Winter  Quarters.”  It  is  a misnomer,  as  the  residents 
are  here  year-round;  but  most  of  them  don’t  go  on  exhibit  during  the  winter  months.  What  many 
“bird  people”  find  amazing  is  that  they  are  now  also  cared  for  not  by  bird  keepers,  but  by  the  same 
keepers  who  take  care  of  their  hoofed  and  homed  neighbors! 


Cinereous  Vulture  {Aegypius  monachus) 


Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5  149 


Our  current  birds  are: 


Male  #81,  “Grimm”,  a parent-reared  bird  from  Palm  Desert,  hatched  in  1999 
Female  #76,  “Cruella”,  hand-reared  at  Birmingham,  hatched  in  1998 

Male  #7 1 , “Triton”,  parent-reared  at  Columbia,  hatched  in  1 996 
Female  #70,  “Poseidon”,  hand-reared  at  Oklahoma,  hatched  in  1996 

Female  #101,  “Eve”,  hand-reared  at  Birmingham,  hatched  in  2006  and  a full  sister  to 
“Cruella”.  She  is  not  currently  paired. 

Cruella  arrived  here  in  1999,  and  was  originally  with  her  brother  “Lurch”  (#73),  who  was  already 
here.  They  were  allowed  to  “grow  up  together”,  but  they  started  showing  a strong  pair  bond  even  at 
a very  young  age.  Cruella  laid  an  egg  in  February  2005,  even  though  they  were  not  set  up  for  nesting. 
The  egg  broke.  Lurch  was  shipped  out  and  replaced  with  Grimm,  who  arrived  in  May  2005.  They 
took  to  each  other  right  away.  At  the  time,  they  shared  the  exhibit  yard  with  a pair  of  Lappet-Faced 
Vultures  (Torgos  tracheliotus)  and  a pair  of  RuppelTs  Griffon  Vultures  {Gyps  rueppellii),  as  well  as 
a young  RuppelTs,  in  addition  to  the  hoofstock. 

The  newly  paired  birds  were  set  up  for  the  breeding  season  in  an  approximately  8’  x 16’  [2.43m 
X 4.87m]  enclosure  with  floor-to-ceiling  walls.  They  were  given  a nesting  platform,  and  everyone 
hoped  for  the  best.  Unfortunately,  as  well  as  they  got  along  in  the  exhibit  yard,  they  did  not  seem  to 
appreciate  the  closer  quarters.  Cruella  would  not  allow  Grimm  to  share  the  nesting  platform,  but  he 
seemed  content  to  stay  on  the  floor  - most  of  the  time.  They  were  given  an  additional  platform  and 
an  extra  stall  space,  and  they  got  along  better  for  the  rest  of  the  winter,  but  did  not  nest. 

They  were  on  exhibit  together  the  summer  of  2006,  and  seemed  to  strengthen  their  pair  bond.  They 
spent  most  of  their  time  together,  often  preening  each  other.  By  the  end  of  summer,  they  were 
presenting  each  other  with  sticks,  and  even  built  a “nest”  in  the  exhibit  yard. 

That  winter,  2006/2007,  they  were  housed  in  a larger  enclosure.  Our  male  zebra  had  died,  and  his 
bam,  approximately  25’  square  [2.32  sq.  m],  was  vacant.  They  were  given  a nesting  platform,  but 
were  not  really  set  up  to  breed.  They  were  not  recommended  that  year  for  genetic  reasons;  thus  they 
were  not  given  the  typical  loads  of  sticks  and  straw  for  nesting.  They  surprised  us  and  laid  an  egg  in 
Febmary  2007,  which  was  found  on  the  ground,  broken  and  partially  eaten.  The  SSP®  recommended 
that  they  be  given  more  (lots  more!)  nesting  material  and  they  re-laid  on  22  March.  Unfortunately, 
that  egg  disappeared  on  31  March. 

Triton  and  Poseidon  arrived  at  the  Milwaukee  County  Zoo  from  the  Racine  Zoo  in  October  2007. 
After  quarantine  at  the  Animal  Health  Center,  they  arrived  in  Winter  Quarters  in  mid-November. 
They  had  been  together  at  Racine  since  1997.  They  had  laid  one  egg  there,  but  it  had  been  broken. 
Since  they  were  so  compatible,  we  set  them  up  in  the  space  that  Grimm  and  Cmella  hadn’t  done  so 
well  with  in  2005/2006.  They  did  get  along  fine,  but  showed  little  interest  in  sticks  or  straw.  Moving 
twice  that  fall  - first  from  Racine  to  Milwaukee,  and  then  from  Quarantine  to  Winter  Quarters,  might 
have  been  a factor. 

Grimm  and  Cmella  spent  their  second  winter  in  the  old  Zebra  Bam.  At  this  point  in  time  we  were 
without  a Curator  of  Birds,  and  Grimm  and  Cmella  did  not  have  a strong  recommendation  to  breed, 
so  they  were  again  not  really  “set  up”  for  breeding.  They  had  a platform,  and  there  was  straw  in  the 
bam,  but  they  were  not  given  much  in  the  line  of  sticks.  Our  new  Curator  started  in  Febmary  of 2008, 
and  he  instmcted  us  to  go  ahead  and  give  them  sticks.  They  were  given  an  armload  of  sticks  at  least 
twice  a week,  and  they  incorporated  them  into  a nest  on  the  ground.  On  26  March,  they  were  sitting 
on  an  egg.  Per  recommendations,  it  was  pulled  on  the  27th  and  replaced  with  a dummy  egg.  Grimm 
and  Cmella  shared  “incubation”  duties  until  late  April,  when  they  were  put  back  on  exhibit  for  the 
summer.  The  real  egg  was  not  artificially  incubated. 

By  this  time,  Cmella’s  sibling.  Eve,  had  also  arrived  at  the  Zoo.  The  five  birds  got  along  well  on 
exhibit.  Although  there  were  interactions  between  all  the  birds,  the  paired  birds  would  generally  be 


150  Animal  Keepers'  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5 


near  each  other  most  of  the  time,  especially  Grimm  and  Cruella.  The  birds  were  brought  in  a little 
earlier  than  usual  that  year  because  of  a colder-than-usual  Fall.  We  also  changed  accommodations 
around  for  the  winter  of  2008/2009. 

Triton  and  Poseidon  got  the  old  Zebra  Bam. 
They  are  the  pair  that  has  the  higher  priority 
to  breed,  so  we  gave  them  the  location 
where  Grimm  and  Cmella  had  laid  eggs 
in  two  years  in  a row.  They  were  given 
two  nesting  platforms,  and  “boatloads” 
of  nesting  materials  beginning  in  mid- 
October. 

Grimm  and  Cmella  were  still  not  highly 
recommended  to  breed,  so  they  were 
housed  in  an  empty  hoofstock  stall,  about 
8’  wide  by  16’  deep  [2.43m  x 4.87m],  with 
one  4’x4’  [1.2m  x 1.2m]  platform.  While 
in  the  past,  we  had  tried  to  set  vultures  up 
in  quiet  locations  with  little  disturbance, 
this  stall  was  across  the  hall  from  a main 
workstation.  As  they  began  nest  building, 
we  realized  that  the  area  was  too  narrow. 
When  they  perched  side  by  side,  one  would 
often  knock  the  other  off  the  perch.  Fortunately,  all  of  our  stall  walls  are  movable,  and  we  merely 
created  a double-wide  stall  for  them,  about  16’  x 16’  [4.87m  x 4.87m]. 

Both  pairs  were  given  sticks  and  straw 
for  nesting.  If  one  has  not  worked  with 
nesting  vultures,  one  cannot  comprehend 
the  quantity  of  sticks  that  these  birds  can 
incorporate  into  a nest  - and  how  many 
more  just  “aren’t  right”  and  don’t  get 
used.  In  the  early  stages  of  nest  building, 
it  seems  somewhat  thicker  sticks  are 
preferred  - about  the  diameter  of  a finger 
or  thumb,  and  2 to  3 feet  in  length  [0.60m 
to  0.91m].  Once  the  foundation  is  set, 
they  seem  to  prefer  a thinner  diameter 
stick,  but  still  want  the  occasional  sturdier 
stick  here  and  there.  We  store  sticks  in 
feedbags,  and  offer  a feedbag-full  twice  a 
week  or  more  once  the  birds  really  start  in 
on  the  nests.  Straw  and  timothy  hay  are 
also  offered,  and  once  the  nest  is  nearing 
completion,  the  birds  like  softer  materials, 
like  long  needled  pine  boughs.  We  have  also  given  them  bamboo  leftover  from  our  Red  Panda 
(Ailurus  fulgens). 

Grimm  and  Cruella  nested  on  their  platform.  Triton  and  Poseidon  started  to  build  one  nest  on  a 
platform,  but  then  decided  to  move  down  to  ground  level. 

One  advantage  to  having  Grimm  and  Cruella  in  a main  work  area  was  that  we  could  hear  - and  see 
- copulations.  By  February  2009,  they  were  copulating  as  often  as  every  70  minutes.  On  5 March, 
they  were  on  an  egg.  Unfortunately,  on  1 1 March,  the  egg  was  gone.  We  assume  one  or  both  of  them 
ate  it,  but  no  shell  fragments  or  any  other  remnants  were  left. 

On  1 7 March  2009,  Triton  and  Poseidon  also  had  an  egg.  Their  egg  was  pulled  and  replaced  with 


Grimm  and  Cruella  in  their  2009/2010  quarters. 
Grimm  is  manipulting  a stick. 


Triton  and  Poseidon  on  a nest  platform 
in  the  Zebra  Bam. 


Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5  151 


a dummy  egg.  The  real  egg  was  taken  to  the  Aviary  where  it  was  artificially  incubated.  The  birds 
returned  to  the  nest  immediately  and  sat  on  their  wooden  egg.  Keepers  noted  that  both  birds  took  !' 
turns  on  the  nest.  ■ 


Grimm  and  Cruella  re-laid  on  8 April; 
this  egg  was  pulled  and  artificially 
incubated,  and  the  birds  were  given  a 
wooden  dummy  egg. 

Both  eggs  were  artificially  incubated  at 
the  Aviary.  Triton  and  Poseidon’s  egg 
tracked  a 13%  weight  loss,  but  Grimm 
and  Cruella’s  tracked  at  8%,  even  though 
the  relative  humidity  in  the  incubator 
was  below  40%.  Vulture  eggs  are  thick 
shelled,  and  are  therefore  difficult  to 
candle.  Even  ultrasound  cannot  always 
determine  viability.  Both  eggs  were 
incubated  full  term,  even  though  no  one 
was  sure  if  they  would  hatch. 

For  reasons  known  only  to  the  birds,  in  late  April  Poseidon  moved  their  dummy  egg  eight  feet 
[2.43m]  along  the  rear  wall  of  the  Zebra  Bam,  and  had  built  a small  nest  up  around  it  there.  The 
egg  was  on  the  bam  floor,  though.  We  moved  the  dummy  egg  back  to  its  original  nest.  The  male 
incubated  it  there;  the  female  sat  on  her  new  but  empty  “nest”.  The  next  day,  she  was  on  the  egg  in 
the  old  nest,  but  the  following  day,  she  had  moved  it  back  to  her  new  spot.  It  was  decided  that  we 
should  let  her  stay  there,  but  give  her  lots  of  material  to  make  the  nest  better. 

Their  real  egg  internally  pipped  in  early  May,  at  49  days.  This  confirmed  that  Triton  and  Poseidon,  at 
approximately  1 3 years  of  age,  were  the  youngest  pair  of  Cinereous  vultures  to  lay  a confirmed  fertile 
egg.  We  planned  on  giving  it  back  to  them  when  it  externally  pipped,  but  it  never  did.  The  chick  was 
declared  dead  in  the  shell  on  10  May  10  at  54  days.  We  hoped  the  birds  would  stay  on  their  dummy 
egg  in  case  we  wanted  to  let  them  raise  Grimm  and  Cruella’s  chick,  to  give  them  practice,  but  by  16 
May,  they  were  no  longer  interested  in  the  nest  at  all.  The  nesting  material  was  removed  from  their 
bam,  they  were  given  their  spring  examinations  and  vaccinations,  and  they  were  put  on  exhibit  for 
the  season. 

Grimm  and  Cmella  were  still  very  attentive  to  their  nest, 
and  on  1 7 May,  it  was  determined  that  their  egg  was  still 
viable.  It  was  returned  to  them  on  20  May  to  let  them 
hatch  it.  Being  only  10  and  11  years  old,  they  had  just 
become  the  youngest  pair  to  have  an  egg  get  to  that  stage! 

On  the  afternoon  of  29  May,  we  could  see  that  there  was 
an  external  pip,  and  by  the  morning  of  the  30th,  the  chick 
had  hatched.  The  pair  seemed  attentive,  and  they  were 
given  extra  feedings.  The  chick  was  seen  moving  during 
one  feeding,  but  we  never  saw  the  parents  feed  the  chick. 

Unfortunately,  the  chick  was  found  apparently  dead  in 
the  nest  the  following  morning.  Grimm  picked  the  chick 
up  and  removed  it  from  the  nest,  jumping  down  to  the 
floor  and  dropping  it.  The  chick  still  had  a large  yolk-sac, 

which  broke  open  on  the  floor.  Neither  adult  bird  showed  any  interest  in  the  dead  chick,  and  did 
not  interfere  with  us  removing  it  from  the  enclosure.  Necropsy  revealed  issues  indicating  a difficult 
hatch,  and  a large,  unabsorbed  yolk  sac,  which  was  either  tom  by  the  parent  or  mptured  when  the 
chick  was  dropped,  but  it  could  not  be  determined  if  the  yolk  sac  broke  before  or  after  the  chick’s 
death. 


Grimm  threatens  as  his  patience 
with  picture  taking  wanes. 


The  beginning  of  Grimm  and  Cruella’s  nest. 


152  Animal  Keepers'  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5 


What  I find  interesting,  overall,  is  that  while  both  our  males  are  parent-reared,  both  our  females 
are  hand-reared,  and  yet  both  pairs  did  everything  they  needed  to  do.  Also,  Grimm  and  Cruella  did 
it  in  a very  “busy”  location,  rather  than  in  the  seclusion  we  normally  think  that  they  require.  It  is 
unfortunate  that  neither  chick  survived,  but  we  are  trying  again  this  year.  Triton  and  Poseidon  have 
the  Zebra  Bam  again,  and  are  already  well  into  nest  building.  Grimm  and  Cmella  are  in  the  area  they 
didn't  use  in  2006,  but  it  has  been  remodeled  to  twice  the  size.  They  are  a few  weeks  behind  the  other 
pair  in  nesting,  but  they  seem  to  be  catching  up.  Wish  us  luck! 

I would  like  to  thank  Ed  Diebold,  Kim  Smith,  and  Alex  Waier,  past  and  present  Curators  of  Birds, 
each  of  whom  has  contributed  to  our  breeding  program,  and  the  other  members  of  the  Winter  Quarters 
staff,  Zookeepers  John  Durrell,  Robert  Collazo,  and  Craig  Pavlik,  who  shared  in  the  responsibilities 
for  the  daily  care  and  monitoring  of  these  magnificent  birds. 


Collaborators  Sought  for  Study 

The  Use  of  Vocal  Playback  as  a Tool  for  Breeding  in  Captive  Birds 

By  Justin  C.  Hickman,  Bird  Keeper 
Tulsa  Zoological  Park,  Tulsa,  OK 

Dr.  Erik  Terdal,  Professor  of  Natural  Science 
Northeastern  State  University 
Tulsa,  OK 

The  study  of  bird  vocalizations  and  how  it  relates 
to  breeding  is  in  its  infancy  with  new  information 
being  discovered  every  day.  This  study  will  relate 
past  research  of  bird  breeding  to  help  increase 
genetic  diversity  in  captive  bird  populations 
found  in  zoos  and  aquariums.  The  purpose  of  this 
research  is  to  study  the  behavior  of  two  captive  bird 
species  in  male/female  enclosures  during  same 
species  vocal  playback.  A pair  of  1.1  Red-and- 
Yellow  Barbets  {Trachyphonus  erythrocephalus) 
and  a pair  of  1 . 1 Plush-Crested  Jays  {Cyanocorax 
chrysops)  will  serve  as  the  subjects  of  observation. 

Each  species  is  housed  on  exhibit  in  single  male/ 
female  enclosures. 

The  study  is  an  observational  study  using  vocal  playbacks  provided  by  Cornell  University  Macaulay 
Library  and  data  will  be  recorded  on  an  ethogram  that  has  been  developed  for  both  individual  species. 
The  hypotheses  is  the  introduction  of  species-specific  vocal  playback  will  have  a positive  affect  on 
breeding  behaviors  in  birds  housed  in  male/female  pairs.  Due  to  the  small  size  of  our  study  group  we 
are  asking  for  interested  institutions  to  participate  so  our  conclusions  will  be  more  valid  and  concrete. 
Any  institutions  that  are  currently  housing  Plush-Crested  Jays  and/or  Red  and  Yellow  Barbets  and  are 
interested  in  participating  please  contact:  Justin  Hickman  by  email  hickman@nusok.edu  or  by  phone 
at  Tulsa  Zoo  - Bird  Department  918-669-6234. 

The  study  will  require  two  months  of  observation  and  would  be  conducted  two  times  a week  with 
each  session  lasting  20  minutes.  All  needed  information  and  vocals  would  be  provided. 


Plush-crested  Jay 
( Photo : ww.tropicalbirding  .com/ } 


Animal  Keepers'  Forum,  VoL  37  No.  4/5  153 


AVIAN  REARING  RESOURCE  WEBSITE 


For  generations  aviculturists  have  been  hand-rearing  birds,  and  over  the  years  protocols 
have  been  refined  and  improved.  However,  this  information  is  not  always  easily  accessible 
and  sometimes  it  is  an  art  (albeit  a time-consuming  one)  to  track  down  the  most  up-to-date 
information.  Only  limited  work  has  been  done  to  measure  the  success  of  these  protocols  and 
the  long-term  effects  on  the  health  and  breeding  success  of  species  that  have  been  hand-reared; 
and  at  a time  when  it  is  vital  that  we  all  work  together  in  an  attempt  to  establish  self-sustaining 
captive  populations,  it  is  more  important  than  ever  that  if  the  decision  is  taken  to  intervene 
and  hand-rear,  it  is  done  with  the  knowledge  that  the  resulting  individuals  will  be  healthy  and 
valuable  additions  to  the  captive  populations. 

Therefore,  a new  website  - www.avianrearingresource.co.uk  - has  been  created  to  compile 
hand-rearing  protocols,  measure  success  rates,  and  highlight  problems  and  the  potential  for 
research  into  improving  protocols,  as  well  as  looking  into  the  long-term  survival  and  breeding 
success  of  hand-reared  birds. 

Aims  of  the  site 

The  aims  of  this  site  are  to  compile  current  hand-rearing  protocols  for  all  species  of  birds,  to 
highlight  any  problems  in  rearing  individual  species,  and  to  make  the  site  accessible  to  all  and 
simple  to  navigate.  Over  time,  it  is  hoped  that  by  sharing  information  we  can  work  together  to 
improve  the  quality  of  hand-reared  birds  and  minimize  mortality.  Using  a rating  system,  the 
success  of  each  protocol  is  measured,  and  there  is  a star-rating  system  for:  the  success  rate, 
whether  the  protocol  has  been  used  successfully  by  two  or  more  institutions/individuals,  points 
and  criteria  for  minimizing  imprinting,  whether  the  birds  go  on  to  produce  fertile  eggs,  and 
whether  the  birds  go  on  to  successfully  parent-rear  their  own  young. 

Site  content 

There  can  be  as  many  as  four  or  five  different  hand-rearing  protocols  for  a single  species,  giving 
users  a better  idea  as  to  which  may  be  the  best  option  for  them.  A protocol  template  can  be  found 
on  the  home  page,  along  with  the  e-mail  contact  for  any  feedback,  etc.  A hand-rearing  decision 
tree  is  included  to  encourage  aviculturists  to  take  the  decision  to  hand-rear  responsibly. 

Additional  information  is  also  included,  such  as  general  information  on  species,  articles  about 
hand-rearing  and  husbandry  guidelines. 

The  site  is  at  present  in  its  infancy  and  with  help  and  input  will  evolve.  All  are  welcome  to 
submit  protocols,  which  along  with  any  comments  or  other  feedback  can  be  sent  to:  Louise  Peat 
at:  avianrearing@googlemail.com. 

Editor’s  Note:  Louise  Peat  is  the  Registrar  at  Cotswold  Wildlife  Park  in  Burford,  Oxfordshire, 
England. 


154  Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5 


Designing  a Keeper  and  Bird-Friendly  Enrichment  Program 

By  Jennifer  Evans,  Lead  Aviculturist 
Tracy  Aviary,  Salt  Lake  City,  Utah 

Many  zoos  have  enrichment  programs  that  look  great  on  paper.  They  have  clear  guidelines  and 
schedules  along  with  documentation  of  the  enrichment  given.  Procedures  are  in  place  to  decide  what 
enrichment  items  are  acceptable  and  how  to  request  permission  to  provide  new  enrichment  items. 
We  had  an  enrichment  program  just  like  that  and  our  keepers  hated  it.  The  program  was  very  time- 
consuming  without  being  effective  at  providing  the  birds  with  meaningfol  enrichment.  The  Aviary 
re-vamped  that  program  to  address  the  individual  needs  of  the  birds  in  our  collection. 

Natural  History 

We  did  keep  several  aspects  of  our  original  program,  the  first  being  researching  the  natural  history  of 
the  species.  The  Tracy  Aviary  has  always  made  it  a goal  to  provide  the  birds  under  our  care  with  as 
many  aspects  of  their  natural  “wild”  lives  as  possible.  Enrichment  is  simply  one  part  of  that.  Keepers 
complete  a Natural  History  form  for  each  species  of  bird  under  their  care.  This  form  looks  at  social 
structure,  wild  habitat,  feeding  strategy,  diet,  activity  cycle,  primary  sensory  modality,  reproductive 
data  and  threats.  This  information  helps  the  keepers  to  understand  more  fiilly  the  life  any  particular 
species  would  be  living  in  the  wild.  They  then  use  that  information  to  compare  the  bird’s  in  situ 
situation  versus  what  we  are  able  to  offer  them  at  the  Aviary. 

Individual  History 

The  second  step  of  our  original  program  was  to  examine  the  individual’s  history  and  housing  situation 
at  the  Aviary.  We  look  at  the  bird’s  exhibit,  medical  history,  behavioral  history,  diet  and  feeding 
schedule,  social  structure,  and  animal/keeper  interactions.  Keepers  would  then  set  enrichment  goals 
based  on  behaviors  they  hoped  to  either  increase  or  decrease.  This  part  of  the  process  at  one  time 
was  simply  a time-consuming  exercise  for  the  keepers  as  it  did  not  play  into  the  enrichment  they  were 
actually  giving  to  the  birds. 

Schedules 

Schedules  were  designed  based  on  species  alone,  not  on  the  individual  Enrichment  was  based 
around  five  categories:  sensory,  foods  and  feeding,  environmental,  manipulative  items/toys,  and 
social/behavioraL  These  categories  had  to  be  rotated  to  ensure  a broad  base  of  enrichment  offerings. 
Birds  that  were  deemed  more  intelligent,  such  as  a corvid  or  parrot,  were  enriched  more  frequently, 
every  two  days,  while  other  birds  such  as  waterfowl  were  enriched  once  a week.  A keeper  may  set  a 
goal  of  increasing  natural  foraging  behaviors,  but  the  program  only  allowed  the  keeper  to  offer  food 
enrichment  a few  times  a month. 

Individual  Guidelines 

The  individual  guidelines  were  developed  to  create  a truly  unique  and  individualized  enrichment 
program  for  each  bird  at  the  Aviary.  While  completing  the  Natural  History  and  Individual  History  is 
a time-consuming  task  for  the  keepers,  the  Individual  Guidelines  creates  a far  less  time-consuming 
enrichment  program.  This  program  not  only  saves  time  but  also  focuses  the  enrichment  on  the  birds 
that  need  it  most.  We  kept  the  five  original  enrichment  categories  and  added  a sixth  category  of 
breeding/nesting.  The  Tracy  Aviary  feels  strongly  that  for  birds,  nesting  is  such  a huge  part  of  their 
life  that  it  cannot  be  ignored.  For  each  individual  bird  or  group  of  birds,  if  they  live  in  the  same 
situation  at  the  Aviary,  we  compare  their  living  situation  to  the  enrichment  categories. 

Sensory 

Sensory  includes  visual,  auditory  and  tactile  and  for  most  birds  olfactory  to  a much  lesser  extent. 
All  animals  use  their  senses  and  birds  are  known  for  their  primary  use  of  sight  and  sound.  Keepers 
examine  the  living  situation  of  the  individual  to  ascertain  the  amount  of  sensory  input  the  bird 
receives  from  its  living  situation.  For  example,  a bird  living  in  an  open  outdoor  exhibit  is  exposed  to 


Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  VoL  37  No.  4/5  155 


the  sights  and  sounds  of  the  entire  Aviary  as  well  as, 
in  many  cases,  the  outside  park  as  well.  A bird  living 
in  a holding  space  sees  the  same  thing  day  in  and  day 
out  and  hears  only  slightly  more. 

Foods  and  Feeding 

The  foods  and  feeding  category  is  self-explanatory. 
Finding  food  is  how  most  birds  spend  a large  part 
of  every  day.  It  is  the  keeper’s  favorite  enrichment 
category.  Give  a bird  its  favorite  treats  and  you  are 
almost  always  guaranteed  a positive  reaction.  The 
keeper’s  propensity  to  do  food  enrichment  requires 
a good  enrichment  program  to  restrict  the  amount 
of  enrichment  food  offered  in  order  to  ensure  the 
bird  is  receiving  a nutritionally  complete  diet.  The 
Tracy  Aviary  decided  to  use  a diet  that  was  almost 
exclusively  made  up  of  pellets  for  most  of  the  birds 
to  ensure  proper  nutrition.  This  opened  up  the  door 
to  being  able  to  allow  keeper’s  to  offer  enrichment 
foods  several  times  a week  on  a rotational  basis.  The  idea  is  simple.  The  birds  are  fed  pellets  in  a 
pan  in  the  morning  when  they  are  most  hungry  and  many  species  naturally  forage  for  food.  Later  in 
the  day  they  are  offered  enrichment  food  items  which  vary  from  bugs  and  meat  to  lettuce  and  seeds 
depending  on  the  species.  These  items  are  offered  in  a manner  that  encourages  the  birds  to  forage. 
This  feeding  style  is  taken  into  account  when  considering  food  enrichment  in  our  overall  enrichment 
plan.  Birds  that  are  on  this  schedule  do  not  require  further  food  enrichment.  Birds  such  as  raptors 
that  are  only  offered  whole  food  items  in  the  same  way  everyday  may  require  food  enrichment. 

Environmental 

Environmental  enrichment  is  anything  that 
changes  the  environment  in  which  the  bird  is 
living.  Wild  birds  change  their  own  environments 
constantly,  simply  by  moving  to  a different 
location.  This  may  mean  walking  or  flying  a few 
feet  away  or  a few  hundred  miles  away.  Captive 
bird’s  environments  don’t  often  change.  There 
isn’t  much  motivation  to  change  an  exhibit  that 
is  well  perched  and  planted.  In  contrast,  birds 
that  live  in  open  outdoor  enclosures  do  see  their 
environment  change.  This  may  come  from  the 
change  in  seasons,  weather,  the  public  or  even 
the  keeper.  Birds  living  in  holding  spaces  see 
almost  no  change  to  their  environment.  For 
example,  while  our  flamingo  flock  must  deal 
with  snow  and  ice  in  the  winter,  going  in  and  out 

of  holding  on  cold  nights,  and  plants  growing  and  weeds  being  removed  in  their  exhibit,  a dove  in 
holding  may  only  have  its  perching  changed  a few  times  while  it  is  in  holding. 

Manipulative  Items/Toys 

This  is  a category  of  enrichment  that  seemed  to  be  largely  wasted  on  many  species  of  birds,  although 
I imagine  heavily  used  for  mammals.  Some  species  like  parrots,  corvids  and  vultures  do  use 
manipulative  enrichment  when  provided.  Many  more  species  don’t  go  near  toys.  In  the  wild  they 
spend  their  days  eating,  preening,  breeding  and  escaping  predators.  Natural  history  is  the  largest 
factor  for  us  in  deciding  how  often  to  enrich  in  this  category. 


Holding  space  with  food  and 
environmental  enrichment. 


{Photo:  Jennifer  Evans) 


Chick  shown  with  foraging  opportunities. 


( Photo:  Jennifer  Evans) 


156  Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5 


Social/Behavioral 

Different  species  of  birds  have  vastly 
different  social  structures.  Some  species 
live  in  flocks  of  thousands,  others  live 
in  small  family  groups,  and  some  live  in 
monogamous  pairs,  while  still  others  live 
alone  outside  of  breeding  season.  Some  live 
with  many  other  species  in  close  proximity 
while  others  live  only  with  conspecifics.  The 
Aviary  attempts  whenever  possible  to  mimic 
these  social  structures.  Good  examples  of 
birds  in  our  collection  that  have  their  social 
needs  well  met  are  the  waterfowl  living  on 
our  main  pond.  They  live  with  conspecifics 
as  well  as  contraspecifics  waterfowl  and 
pelicans.  In  addition  to  the  birds  exhibited 
with  them  they  also  have  wild  passerines, 
waterfowl,  and  even  predators  visiting  their 
pond.  Waterfowl  in  holding  obviously  do  not  have  all  of  these  social  opportunities  and  may  require 
additional  enrichment. 

Breeding/Nesting 

Once  a bird  is  breeding  that  becomes  a singular  focus  for  them.  The  Tracy  Aviary  does  not  breed 
every  bird  in  the  collection,  but  we  do  provide  every  bird  with  the  opportunity  to  go  through  the 
experience  - short  of  chick  rearing.  Breeding  and  enrichment  are  linked  in  different  ways  for  different 
species  and  individuals.  For  a pair  of  swans  providing  them  enrichment  while  they  are  building 
and  defending  a nest  is  unnecessarily 
disruptive  to  their  breeding  behavior, 
especially  if  you  wish  to  have  them 
rear  offspring.  During  this  period  we 
focus  all  enrichment  on  providing 
them  with  more  nesting  materials  and 
abundant  food  sources.  For  a pair  of 
corvids  providing  intensive  enrichment 
including,  but  not  limited  to,  food 
enrichment  can  encourage  them  to 
breed  or  deter  them  from  destroying 
their  own  eggs  or  offspring. 

Schedule 

Once  the  keepers  have  compared  each 
category  of  enrichment  to  the  natural 
history  of  the  bird  and  its  individual 
living  situation  they  can  make  a 
schedule  of  enrichment  for  the  bird.  Many  of  our  birds  living  in  groups  or  pairs  in  outdoor  exhibits 
have  most  of  their  enrichment  needs  met.  We  know  those  needs  are  met  so  we  are  able  to  focus  our 
enrichment  time  on  birds  in  holding  spaces  or  birds  housed  alone  that  do  not  have  many  of  their 
enrichment  needs  met.  This  plan  allows  keepers  more  time  to  provide  higher  quality  enrichment  to 
the  birds  that  really  need  it. 

Next  Steps 

This  enrichment  plan  is  in  its  early  stages  at  the  Tracy  Aviary.  The  initial  research  is  quite  time- 
intensive so  the  going  is  slow.  The  Aviary  is  starting  an  Enrichment  Volunteer  program  to  help  the 
keepers  in  their  research  of  bird’s  natural  history,  the  creation  and  preparation  of  enrichment  items 
and  most  importantly  the  observation  of  the  birds  after  they  have  received  the  enrichment.  The  extra 
help  will  hopefully  allow  keepers  to  focus  on  creating  very  individualized  enrichment  programs  for 
the  birds  under  their  care. 


Birds  in  yard  with  manipulative  enrichment. 


{Photo:  by  Rebecca  Bates) 


Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5  157 


Successfully  Raising  Parent-reared  Chicks 
from  Elegant  Crested  Tinamou  (Eudromia  elegans) 

By  Reade  Harbitter,  Animal  Keeper 
Smithsonian's  National  Zoo,  Washington,  D.C. 

In  October  2009  we  welcomed  our  first  successful  Elegant  Crested  Tinamou  chicks  hatched  at 
Smithsonian’s  National  Zoo.  The  Bird  House  has  been  home  to  tinamou  for  many  years;  however 
we  have  not  had  a successful  hatching  until  this  year.  The  dam  arrived  at  the  zoo  only  five  months 
ago  and  the  sire  is  a first-time  father.  Elegant  crested  tinamous  {Eudromia  elegans)  are  smallish 
ground-dwelling  birds  native  to  Argentina  and  Chile.  They  have  a compact  build,  with  a small  head, 
decurved  bill,  and  grey  and  brown  streaked  plumage.  As  adults,  the  birds  typically  posses  a long 
crest,  normally  carried  backwards  but  raised  forwards  when  excited.  They  are  at  home  in  arid  and 
semi-arid  grassland,  dry  savanna,  open  woodland,  and  dry  Andean  steppes.  There  are  approximately 
47  species  of  tinamous  ranging  across  Central  and  South  America.  Most  tinamous  are  not  globally 
threatened  while  some  species  are  critically  endangered.  All  tinamou  suffer  from  population  decline 
due  to  habitat  loss  and  hunting  for  food  (Del  Hoyo,  1992). 


There  are  90  Elegant  crested  tinamous 
housed  in  18  institutions  across  the  U.S. 
The  studbook  data  indicates  that  38  chicks 
were  hatched  in  2009  and  of  those  1 1 were 
parent-reared.  Not  much  information  is 
available  on  parent-reared  tinamous  or 
Elegant  crested  tinamous  in  particular, 
however  hopefully  the  techniques  used  at 
the  National  Zoo  may  be  helpful  to  any 
other  institution  wishing  to  attempt  parent- 
reared  chicks. 


The  male  tinamou  keeps  close  watch  on  two  of  his  ^he  breeding  season  for  Elegant  crested 
j ^ ^ ^ tinamous  IS  Similar  to  that  of  Other  tinamous, 

June/ August  - November,  with  most  laying 
occurring  during  October.  Simultaneous  polygamy  and  sequential  polyandry  is  typical,  with  the 
female  laying  5-9  olive  or  yellowish  shiny  green  eggs.  Incubation  typically  lasts  20-21  days  and 
is  carried  out  completely  by  the  male.  This  is  similar  to  rheas  and  other  ratites  (Struthioniformes) 
which  are  thought  to  be  closely  related  to  the  tinamous  (Tinamiformes).  Mortality  rates  are  very  high 
in  the  wild,  and  in  the  first  few  days  of  life  chicks  are  especially  susceptible  to  predation  from  wild 
cats,  skunks,  foxes,  and  raptors  (Del  Hoyo,  1992). 


The  male  and  female  were  introduced  to  each  other  in  late  July  2009  and  after  acclimating  together 
for  several  months,  were  subsequently  housed  in  a mixed-species  exhibit  visible  to  the  public.  Also 
in  the  exhibit  were  1 . 1 Yellow  breasted  ground  dove  {Gallicolumba  tristigmata),  0. 1 Blue  winged  teal 
{Anas  discors),  0.1  Ringed  teal  {Callonetta  leucoprus),  and  1.1  Fairy  blue  birds  {Irena  puella).  The 
National  Zoo  has  kept  pairs  of  Elegant  crested  tinamous  in  the  past,  but  has  never  successfully 
hatched  chicks.  This  particular  male  has  been  with  other  females  in  different  exhibits,  but  has  never 
exhibited  consistent  incubation  behavior  and  eggs  never  made  it  full  term. 

Parent-raised  chick  mortality  is  generally  believed  to  be  very  high  among  Elegant  crested  tinamous 
(K.  Clark,  pers.  Commun.).  These  birds  are  often  kept  in  multi-species  exhibits,  so  this  may  be  due 
to  aggression  or  predation  from  other  species  as  well  as  parent  competition  or  aggression.  The  pair 
at  National  Zoo  was  approved  for  breeding  by  the  studbook  keeper,  so  it  was  decided  to  go  ahead  and 
allow  the  male  to  raise  the  chicks  but  with  no  expectations. 


158  Animal  Keepers'  Forum,  VoL  37  No.  4/5 


Beginning  in  late  September  2009,  copulation  was  observed  several  times  by  animal  care  staff.  The 
first  shiny  green  egg  was  laid  on  1 October  in  a small,  leaf-lined  depression  in  the  gravel  under  a 
plant  in  the  comer  of  the  exhibit.  The  male  spent  most  of  his  time  on  the  nest,  leaving  periodically 
to  eat  or  drink  and  then  returning.  Sheets  of  black  plastic  were  placed  in  between  the  nest  and  the 
public  area  to  give  the  male  some  privacy.  As  is  typical  of  tinamous,  he  remained  motionless  on  the 
nest  and  was  extremely  tolerant  of  keepers’  approach.  Animal  care  staff  could  approach  very  closely 
and  even  touch  the  incubating  male  without  causing  him  to  leave  the  nest.  Incubating  tinamou  males 
have  been  known  to  feign  injury  and  perform  dramatic  distraction  displays  when  frightened  off  the 
nest,  however  this  behavior  was  never  observed  in  this  male.  Six  more  eggs  were  laid  over  a period 
of  2 1 days,  and  upon  candling  all  but  two  were  found  to  be  fertile.  The  infertile  eggs  were  discarded 
and  the  four  fertile  ones  were  replaced  on  the  nest,  including  one  with  a small  crack. 

To  minimize  the  effects  of  hand-rearing,  staff  decided  on  paternal  incubation  despite  the  risks  of 
chick  mortality.  The  male  appeared  to  be  incubating  very  well  which  was  promising. 

The  female  laid  two  more  eggs,  17  and  28  days  after  the  first,  and  with  six  eggs  now  in  the  small 
nest  it  was  starting  to  mn  out  of  room.  By  late  October,  the  eggs  were  approaching  the  estimated 
hatch  date  so  staff  began  preparations  for  the  chicks.  The  female  was  moved  to  the  exhibit  next 
door,  and  more  black  plastic  was  placed  in  between  the  two  exhibits  so  that  the  male  would  not  be 
distracted  by  seeing  the  female  in  close  proximity.  The  small  pool  was  dropped  and  replaced  with 
shallow  water  pans  lined  with  small  rocks.  The  small  plastic  rock  containers  that  conceal  mousetraps 
were  also  removed  from  this  exhibit  as  well  as  the  adjoining  exhibit.  On  2 September  the  remaining 
animals  were  removed  from  the  exhibit  leaving  only  the  incubating  male  and  the  1.1  Fairy  bluebirds 

that  were  not  considered  a threat  to  the 
hatching  chicks.  Keepers  and  Commissary 
made  plans  to  provide  the  chicks  with  ratite 
starter  pellets  and  finely  chopped  greens, 
while  discontinuing  live  insects  in  that  exhibit 
and  the  adjoining  one  until  the  chicks  were 
larger.  Three  eggs  had  pipped  by  the  end  of 
the  day  on  30  October,  and  four  chicks  hatched 
on  3 1 October  including  the  cracked  egg.  The 
remaining  two  eggs  were  determined  to  be 
late-term  deaths.  Initially  the  male  spent  most 
of  the  day  hiding  the  chicks  under  his  body 
and  wings;  the  chicks  made  a few  forays  into 
the  exhibit  without  straying  too  far  from  the 
male. 

The  four  chicks  were  seen  eating  and 
drinking  from  their  pans  within  two  days  of 
hatching.  Newly-hatched  chicks  are  capable 
of  feeding  themselves,  however  it  is  common 
for  the  male  to  find  food  or  insects  and  place  it  near  the  chicks  for  the  first  few  days.  The  group 
was  given  three  separate  feedings  (0800,  1130,  and  1430  hrs)  with  the  food  divided  between  two 
shallow  pans.  If  a paternal  fecal  was  found,  it  was  scooped  and  placed  into  the  chicks’  dishes  to 
promote  natural  ingestion.  Cleaning  in  the  exhibit  was  kept  at  a minimum  for  the  first  week,  with 
no  hosing  and  minimal  disturbances.  Access  was  restricted  to  necessary  personnel  only  to  avoid  any 
accidents.  The  four  chicks  appeared  healthy  and  continued  to  develop  normally.  After  a week  the 
birds  began  to  receive  wax  worms  {Galleria  mellonella)  and  were  spending  more  time  away  from 
the  male.  After  two  weeks  regular  exhibit  maintenance  was  resumed.  The  pool  (approximately  6-8” 
deep/1 5.3-20.5cm)  was  refilled  and  rodent  control  resumed.  The  male  began  to  spend  more  time 
away  from  the  chicks  and  show  some  mild  aggression  towards  keepers:  pecking  at  shoes  or  pants 
legs,  following  them  around  the  exhibit,  etc.  but  this  behavior  had  been  normal  for  this  animal  before 


This  young  Elegant  Crested  Tinamou  is  four  days 

old.  {Photo:  Mehgan  Murphy,  Smithsonian’s  National  Zoo) 


Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5  159 


the  eggs  were  laid.  Tinamou  chicks  are  capable  of  flying  short  distances  at  one  to  three  weeks  old, 
however  this  behavior  was  not  observed. 

By  12  December  the  chicks  had  grown  considerably  and  were  now  receiving  two  feedings  daily 
(OSOOhrs  and  1430hrs).  The  Ringed  teal  and  Blue-winged  teal  were  returned  to  the  exhibit  and  the 
visual  barrier  separating  the  sire  from  the  dam  was  partially  removed.  The  ducks  and  the  tinamous 
were  not  observed  interacting  but  did  remain  on  separate  sides  of  the  enclosure.  As  of  26  December, 
all  four  chicks  are  doing  well  and  are  being  transitioned  to  ratite  pellets  in  one  pan  that  is  fed  in 
the  morning  and  remains  throughout  the  day.  The  chicks  are  similar  in  size  to  the  adults  but  not  as 
filled  out  in  body  weight.  Some  species  of  tinamou  can  achieve  adult  size  within  20  days;  however 
achieving  adult  weight  can  take  between  85  to  108  days  in  Elegant  crested  tinamou.  The  female 
is  still  separated  from  the  male  and  four  chicks,  and  will  most  likely  remain  so  until  the  chicks  are 
moved  to  another  exhibit  or  another  zoo. 

Bird  House  staff  is  fortunate  to  have  achieved  100%  chick  survival  rate  in  our  parent-reared  tinamou. 
We  attribute  our  success  to  providing  the  sire  with  privacy  and  distraction  from  exhibit  mates,  visual 
distraction  from  the  dam,  and  a bit  of  luck!  We’ve  all  learned  a lot  watching  these  tinamou  grow  up, 
and  hopefully  any  future  attempts  will  go  just  as  smoothly. 


References 

Del  Hoyo,  Josep,  A.  Elliot,  and  J.  Sargatal.  1992.  Handbook  of  the  birds  of  the  world. 
Lynx  Edicions,  Spain.  1:112-138. 

Personal  communications  with  Kristen  Clark,  Elegant  Crested  Tinamou  Studbook  Keeper 
at  Smithsonian’s  National  Zoo. 


Acknowledgements 

Thanks  to  Sara  Hallager,  Biologist  at  Smithsonian’s  National  Zoo. 


Big  Cat  Internships  Available 

Join  US  in 

“Saving  Tigers  One  by  One” 


As  seen  on  Animal  Planet® 
“Growing  Up  Tiger” 

Apply  at:  www.tigercreek.org 


Leam  about  Big  Cat  Management.  Internship  involves  Animal  Care  Apprentice  and 
Public  Education.  We  offer  experience  that  counts  towards  employment. 

TIGER  MISSING  LINK  FOUNDATION  / TIGER  CREEK  WILDLIFE  REFUGE 


160  Animal  Keepers'  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5 


Ratite  Behavioral  Husbandry  at  the  Philadelphia  Zoo 


By  Wendy  Lenhart,  Bird  Keeper  and  Ellen  Bartuska,  Mammal  Keeper 
The  Philadelphia  Zoo,  Philadelphia,  PA 


Ratites  can  pose  a challenge  to  animal  care  professionals  due  to  their  large  size.  While  these  animals 
can  be  restrained  and  sedated  for  routine  veterinary  procedures,  voluntary  cooperation  for  these 
husbandry  practices  has  enduring  health  benefits.  Building  positive  relationships  with  collection 
animals  allows  short  or  long-term  medications  to  be  administered  with  high  success  rates  and 
desensitizes  the  birds  to  keeper  presence  so  that  necessary  exhibit  maintenance  does  not  induce  stress. 
At  the  Philadelphia  Zoo,  keepers  have  used  training  for  the  husbandry  of  an  elderly  female  Emu 
{Dromaius  novaehollandiae)  and  a pair  of  female  Ostrich  (Struthio  camelus)  with  great  success. 

In  the  winter  of  2007,  a 23-year-old  female  emu,  “Mrs.  Emu”  at  the  Philadelphia  Zoo  exhibited 
a limp  that  was  causing  her  to  stumble  and  get  abrasions  on  her  left  foot.  Staff  suspected  arthritis 
was  causing  the  discomfort  and  anti-inflammatory  medication  was  prescribed.  The  Emu  quickly 
began  hand-feeding  favored  food  items  like  apple  chunks  and  ratite  pellets  from  a bowl  held  by 
the  keeper.  Medication  was  successfully  disguised  in  this  food  and  delivered  to  the  bird  in  this 
manner.  Additionally,  keepers  were  able  to  take  advantage  of  their  good  relationship  with  the  bird  to 
administer  a warm  compress  to  the  irritated  area  on  her  left  leg.  Once  weather  conditions  warmed  in 
the  spring,  keepers  started  targeting  her  to  approach  from  across  the  yard  for  shifting  purposes.  The 
cue  was  a vocal  “come  here”  and  she  was  rewarded  by  grabbing  an  apple  chunk  from  the  keeper’s 
flattened  hand.  After  each  training  session,  Mrs.  Emu  was  given  the  bulk  of  her  ratite  pellets  as  a 
“jackpot’  reward.  Often  this  was  given  when  she  needed  to  shift  indoors  overnight. 

Though  the  Emu  was  being  treated  successfully  with  oral  medication  for  her  arthritis,  keeper  staff 
and  veterinary  staff  wanted  to  x-ray  the  affected  area  of  her  leg.  Because  of  her  large  size  and 
advanced  age,  it  was  not  feasible  to  move  the  bird  to  the  animal  hospital  for  the  procedure.  The 

plan  was  instead  to  train  the  bird  to 
walk  up  a ramp  to  elevate  her  feet 
off  the  ground  and  snap  images  with 
a portable  x-ray  machine.  The  ramp 
used  was  36”x  36”x  8”  [.914m  x 
.914m  X .20m]  with  a 45°  incline. 
The  bird  was  desensitized  to  the  ramp 
by  first  placing  her  food  bowl  on  it 
as  though  it  was  a feeding  station. 
Eventually  keepers  baited  her  to  walk 
up  it  until  she  would  wait  at  the  top 
before  receiving  a food  reward.  Her 
food  reward  was  modified  to  an  entire 
bowl  of  ratite  pellets  and  chunks  of 
apple  to  keep  her  distracted  during 
the  entire  procedure.  This  training 
took  several  weeks  to  complete  and 
was  embedded  into  her  daily  routine. 
Additionally,  training  sessions  were 
done  with  the  keeper  wearing  an 
x-ray  apron  and  holding  an  x-ray  plate  to  desensitize  the  animal  to  these  objects.  The  x-ray  plate  was 
fastened  to  a broom  handle  so  that  it  could  be  positioned  from  a safe  distance  (see  photo). 

The  first  attempt  to  do  the  voluntary  x-ray  procedure  with  veterinary  staff  was  only  partially 


(Photo  by  Amy  Ivins) 


Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5  161 


successful  because  the  bird  became  nervous  and  wary  of  the  x-ray  machine  and  the  length  of  time 
it  was  taking  for  set-up.  However,  she  did  step  in  place  as  trained  and  tolerated  the  addition  of  two 
extra  people  in  the  yard  prior  to  the  introduction  of  the  machine.  The  following  day  the  process  was 
greatly  sped  up  and  x-ray  pictures  confirming  arthritis  in  the  left  leg  were  taken.  The  now  25-year-old 
Emu  is  still  on  anti-inflammatory  medication  and  is  still  hand-fed,  though  she  has  been  retired  from 
her  ramp-climbing.  She  is  active  and  has  a good  quality-of-life  due  to  the  successful  management  of 
her  condition. 

During  the  summer  of  2008,  the  Philadelphia  Zoo  added  two  young  adult  female  Ostriches  to  the 
collection.  They  were  estimated  to  be  approximately  three-years-old  and  had  come  from  a large 
farm  herd.  After  some  initial  challenges  with  administering  their  de-wormer  in  quarantine,  animal 
department  staff  determined  that  a basic  training  program  instituting  target-feeding  was  in  their  best 
interest.  Keepers  did  not  encounter  much  initial  aggression  from  these  birds,  but  rather  apprehension. 
To  encourage  bonding  with  their  keepers,  a program  of  hand-feeding  was  instituted.  The  birds  had 
never  been  weighed,  so  scale  training  was  an  additional  identified  goal.  It  became  apparent  that  a 1 : 1 
bird  to  trainer  ratio  would  be  ideal.  This  posed  some  staffing  challenges  so  a keeper  from  a different 
animal  building  assisted  in  the  twice  daily  training  sessions.  In  this  case,  mammal  department  keepers 
in  the  “African  Plains”  area  at  the  zoo  were  responsible  for  the  primary  care  of  the  Ostriches,  but  the 
Emu  keeper  from  the  bird  department  provided  additional  training  support. 

The  Ostriches  “Ethyl”  and 
“Lucy”  were  each  assigned  a 
specific  trainer  who  hand-fed 
rathe  pellets  from  an  individually 
colored  three-quart  feeder 
scoop  purchased  from  a Nasco® 
agricultural  supply  catalogue. 

At  the  onset  of  training,  the 
majority  of  the  birds’  pellets 
were  offered  during  two  daily 
training  sessions.  Whatever 

diet  was  left  unconsumed  at  the 
end  of  the  day  was  given  ad  lib 
overnight.  The  scoops  were  large 
enough  to  protect  each  trainer 
from  a wayward  beak  and  were 
conspicuous  to  the  birds.  These 
interactions  began  at  a fence  dividing  the  animals  from  the  people,  but  progressed  to  a point  of  open 
contact  in  the  yard.  Within  a week,  each  ostrich  would  approach  “their”  keeper  with  “their”  colored 
scoop  with  little  to  no  hesitation  and  the  animals  could  be  easily  shifted.  It  is  important  to  note  that 
some  training  foods  were  offered  to  these  birds  initially  including  apples,  grapes,  and  pinky  mice,  but 
the  birds  were  only  interested  in  Mazuri®  ratite  pellets. 

After  the  success  with  hand-feeding  and  baiting  the  animals  across  the  yard,  target  sticks  were 
introduced.  These  were  made  with  1/2”  x 24”  [1.27cm  x ~61cm]  wooden  dowels  with  a three-inch 
[7.62cm]  diameter  plastic  ball  affixed  at  one  end.  As  with  the  scoops,  each  bird  was  assigned  a 
colored  target.  Keepers  would  present  the  target  to  their  Ostrich  while  holding  the  feeder  scoop 
behind  their  back  with  the  opposite  hand  (see  photo).  Once  Ethyl  or  Lucy  pecked  their  ball,  the 
scoop  was  brought  out  for  a mouthful  of  pellet  reward.  Target  stick  retreated  to  behind  each  trainer’s 
back  until  the  next  cue  to  move  was  to  be  given.  It  was  important  that  the  plastic  balls  were  secure 
on  the  dowels  because  the  birds  became  quite  zealous  with  the  process.  They  learned  the  targeting 
behavior  within  a few  days  and  enthusiastically  approached  keepers  at  the  beginning  of  each  training 
session.  It  was  also  noted  that  before  the  initiation  of  training,  the  ostriches  would  leave  some  of 
their  pellet  diet  at  the  end  of  the  day,  or  lose  a proportion  of  it  to  wild  birds.  By  the  time  the  targeting 


(Photo  by  Marianne  Lubenow) 


162  Animal  Keepers'  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5 


behaviors  were  cemented,  the  majority  of  the  pellets  were  consumed  by  the  pair  during  training 
sessions  reducing  waste  and  allowing  for  better  monitoring  of  food  consumption. 

Target  training  the  Ostrich  pair  gave  the  flexibility  needed  for  positioning  the  birds  for  their  first 
voluntary  weights.  An  Arlyn®  320- Vet  scale  was  placed  between  two  hoofstock  stalls  adjacent  to 
the  indoor  ostrich  holding.  The  stall  door  separating  the  two  stalls  was  closed  just  enough  to  allow 
passage  of  a single  bird.  Keepers  worked  the  birds  from  one  stall  to  the  other  through  the  narrow 
gap  and  eventually  placed  the  scale  in  the  gap  so  that  the  birds  had  to  pass  over  it.  Both  birds  were 
initially  nervous  with  the  new  surface  and  would  place  a single  foot  on  it  or  not  remain  still  long 
enough  for  the  weight  to  register.  After  another  several  days  of  steady  repetition,  nervousness  abated 
and  weights  were  finally  recorded  for  both  birds. 

The  intensity  of  the  early  training  has  since  diminished  but  the  pair  still  reliably  hand-feeds,  shifts, 
and  takes  oral  medication  when  needed.  There  is  also  reason  to  believe  that  Ethyl  and  Lucy  were 
able  to  discern  the  difference  in  color  between  their  feeder  scoops  and  targets.  On  several  occasions 
after  the  major  training  milestones  had  be  reached,  keepers  switched  birds,  and  used  the  individual 
bird’s  same  colored  targets  and  scoop  to  rule  out  trainer  bias.  On  another  occasion,  the  birds  were 
separated  from  each  other,  and  one  trainer  offered  two  different  colored  target  choices  to  each  bird. 
One  ball  was  the  color  to  which  the  bird  was  originally  trained  and  the  second  was  the  color  to  which 
the  alternate  bird  was  trained.  With  a very  high  success  rate,  the  birds  were  able  to  discern  and  target 
to  their  “correct”  colored  ball.  Keepers  hope  to  run  some  color-choice  trials  in  the  near  future  for 
fiirther  research. 

Interdepartmental  cooperation  played  an  immense  role  in  the  success  of  the  ratite  training  that 
has  taken  place  at  the  Philadelphia  Zoo.  Open  dialogue  between  keeper,  curatorial,  nutrition,  and 
veterinary  staff  was  invaluable  when  assessing  animal  needs  and  making  important  training  steps. 
A special  thanks  goes  out  to  Paul  Kalka,  primary  relief  and  secondary  trainer  to  “Mrs  Emu”  as  well 
as  veterinary  staff  Dr.  Chris  Montgomery,  Dr.  Donna  laleggio.  Dr.  Keith  Hinshaw,  Martha  Vaca  and 
Sue  Isackson.  Thanks  also  to  Dr.  Aliza  Baltz,  Christine  Bartos,  Kim  Lengel,  Betsy  Karkowski,  and 
Lynn  Tunmer. 

Notes; 

Ratite  Training  Goals  for  Behavioral  Husbandry  at  the  Philadelphia  Zoo 

,/  - Osbricft  Trainmg, -Goals 

1 . Reliably  get  the  emu  to  hand-feed  to 
establish/reinforce  a positive  relationship 
and  voluntarily  deliver  medication 

2.  Target  the  emu  to  approach  the  keeper 
on  cue  to  facilitate  shifting 

3.  Desensitize  the  emu  to  touch  to  apply 
warm  compresses  or  other  necessary 
topical  medications 

4.  Train  the  emu  to  station  for  a voluntary 
x-ray  using  a portable  x-ray  machine  in  her 
outdoor  yard 


Author^s  Addendum:  “Mrs.  Emu”  died  on  25  January  2010,  less  than  a month  short  of  her  26th 
birthday.  She  was  an  intelligent  and  gentle  animal  who  will  be  sorely  missed  by  those  who  had  the 
opportunity  to  work  with  her. 


1.  Reliably  get  the  ostriches  to  hand-feed  to 
establish/reinforce  a positive  relationship 
and  voluntarily  deliver  routine  medication 

2.  Target  train  the  ostriches  to  approach 
keeper  staff  on  cue  to  facilitate  shitting 

3.  Fine-tune  target  training  to  position  the 
ostriches  on  a scale  for  voluntary  weighing 


Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5  163 


Frogmouth  Management  at  Sea  World  Orlando 
and  Discovery  Cove 


By  Liane  Berlepsch,  Senior  1 Aviculturist 
SeaWorld  Orlando,  Orlando,  Florida 


Introduction 

Tawny  frogmouths  {Podargus  strigoides)  belong  to  the  order  Caprimulgiformes.  They  are  often 
mistaken  for  owls  because  of  their  similar  appearance.  They  resemble  owls  with  their  large  eyes  and 
body  shape  and  are  nocturnal  and  silent  in  flight.  Unlike  owls  that  hunt  with  their  talons,  Tawny 
frogmouths  hunt  with  their  mouths.  Prey  is  either  caught  on  the  ground  or  they  sit  and  wait  for  prey 
to  come  to  them,  using  the  feathery  vibrissae  around  their  bills  to  direct  insects  into  their  mouths. 
Frogmouths  are  native  to  Australia,  coastal  islands  and  Tasmania.  They  are  found  in  heavy  forests, 
but  are  also  common  in  suburban  areas.  Being  nocturnal  and  solitary,  they  are  rarely  seen. 

The  breeding  of  frogmouths  at  SeaWorld  Orlando  has  always  been  encouraged  and  there  was  a burst 
of  reproduction  in  2000  from  a very  prolific  pair.  Today,  we  have  five  pair  set-up  for  breeding  and  in 
the  past  24  months  have  had  chicks  or  eggs  from  all  five.  Care,  exhibit  set-up  and  hand-rearing  of  the 
chicks  has  involved  the  entire  Aviculture  staff  from  both  SeaWorld  and  Discovery  Cove. 

Since  2000,  the  North  American  population  of  Tawny  frogmouths  has  declined  by  29%.  Over  the  last 
decade  (1999-2009),  54  frogmouth  hatchings  have  occurred  and  22  of  those  hatches  (41%)  occurred 
at  Sea  World  Orlando.  An  overall  35%  chick  mortality  (death  at  <30  days  of  age)  has  been  observed 
during  this  ten-year  period.  By  employing  and  refining  hand-rearing  techniques  for  the  species, 
SeaWorld  Orlando  has  increased  the  survival  (<30-day)  rate  of  chicks  by  12%.  The  current  intense 
program  will  continue  until  the  North  American  population  becomes  stable.  At  that  time,  breeding 
pairs  may  be  relocated  to  other  institutions  to  free  up  space  for  additional  species.  The  long-term 
sustainability  of  captive  bird  populations  has  been  the  subject  of  much  discussion  over  the  last  five 
years.  A steady  and  sustained  decline  in  the  North  American  Tawny  frogmouth  population,  coupled 
with  a low  reproductive  rate,  prompted  the  Population  Management  Program  (PPM®)  to  identify 
captive-breeding  as  the  program’s  top  priority.  SeaWorld  Orlando  recognized  this  goal  and  devoted 
important  off-exhibit  space  to  work  with  multiple  breeding  pairs.  Genetically  important  frogmouths, 
which  were  otherwise  being  utilized  for  Education/Presentation  purposes  within  the  SeaWorld 
organization,  were  re-allocated  for  breeding  purposes  and  other  frogmouths  identified  through  the 
PMP®  were  brought  into  the  collection  in  2007  (Myers,  2009) 

Enclosure 

Tawny  frogmouths  in  our  collection  have  been  housed  in  a variety  of  habitats.  Due  to  their  docile 
nature,  they  can  be  exhibited  with  almost  any  other  species  of  bird.  They  have  thrived  in  free-flight 
aviaries,  large  walk-through  exhibits  and  smaller  aviaries  on-  and  off-display.  In  the  past,  we  have 
seen  successful  breeding  by  certain  pairs  regardless  of  where  they  were  housed.  However,  with  the 
recent  commitment  to  breed  frogmouths,  every  attempt  has  been  made  to  isolate  pairs  in  their  own 
aviaries  that  offer  the  least  amount  of  distraction.  These  off-exhibit  areas  are  fairly  simple,  wire- 
framed aviaries  of  different  sizes  with  substrates  of  concrete  or  mulch.  Foliage  ranges  from  a single 
potted  palm  to  a fully  planted  aviary.  All  are  outdoors  and  offer  an  ample  amount  of  flying  room. 
Aviaries  are  perched  in  such  a way  that  natural  light  can  reach  the  birds,  as  frogmouths  love  to  sun 
themselves.  Perches  don’t  need  to  be  plentiful,  but  varied  in  height.  Some  birds  like  to  perch  as  high 
as  the  rafters;  others  prefer  to  be  at  lower  levels. 

Diet 

Historically,  frogmouth  diets  have  been  offered  by  hand  rather  than  from  a food  pan.  In  the  past. 


164  Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5 


the  birds  were  fed  twice  a day  with  staff  going  out  of  their  way,  even  climbing  trees,  attempting  to 
feed  them.  With  experience  came  the  realization  that  frogmouths  do  well  on  one  feeding  a day  and 
our  procedure  changed  to  hand-feeding  only  in  the  mornings,  until  the  birds  were  satiated.  It  is  not 
uncommon  for  some  birds  to  refuse  food  for  days  and  still  thrive.  Mice,  typically  peach  fuzzy  and 
hopper  sizes,  bugs,  including  frozen  crickets  (thawed  out)  and  mighty  mealworms,  were  given  as 
part  of  their  daily  diet.  Most  birds  have  their  preferences  on  the  size  rodent  they  will  accept;  some 
will  even  refiise  the  larger  mice,  preferring  only  the  smaller  sizes.  Some  Tawny  frogmouths  can  be 
enthusiastic  feeders,  making  it  almost  impossible  to  hand  feed  bugs.  They  snap  at  the  food  items  and 
sometimes  get  a finger  instead.  These  birds  can  be  fed  using  large  tongs  to  prevent  this.  Even  without 
the  bugs,  the  rodent  diet,  sprinkled  with  calcium  carbonate  and  Vionate®  in  a 50:50  ratio,  is  sufficient. 
For  enrichment,  Tawnys  will  hunt  live  crickets  offered  by  the  aviculturists.  Appetites  fluctuate  often, 
even  daily.  Some  birds  may  be  hunting  at  night  in  their  aviaries  without  us  knowing,  which  would 
explain  their  appetite  flux.  However,  it  is  important  to  be  familiar  with  the  birds’  eating  habits.  Where 
it  may  not  be  unusual  to  go  off  food  for  some  individuals,  it  could  be  concerning  for  others.  Keep 
in  mind  it  is  not  uncommon  for  adults  to  refuse  eating  at  times,  especially  when  they  are  handled  or 
moved  to  a new  area.  Along  the  same  lines,  weights  will  also  fluctuate  throughout  the  year. 


Adult  Male/Femafe  Average  Weight  Comparison 


Male 

Female 


Because  our  frogmouths  were  hand  feeders,  they  were  never  given  the  opportunity  to  raise  their  own 
offspring.  This  year  a plan  of  action  was  put  into  place  to  modify  their  eating  habits  and  condition 
them  to  eat  from  a pan.  We  wanted  them  to  be  self-sufficient  and  no  longer  rely  on  the  aviculturists 
as  their  source  of  food.  Before  the  process  was  implemented,  all  birds  were  weighed  and  then 
monitored  closely  during  the  transition  period.  Hand-feeding  was  eliminated  right  away.  The  birds 
are  most  active  in  the  early  morning  before  the  sun  comes  up,  and  diets  were  offered  at  this  time. 
Food  items  were  placed  in  a shallow  pan  and  held  in  front  of  the  birds  so  they  could  help  themselves. 
Using  live  mealworms  stimulated  them  to  lunge  for  the  food,  so  a healthy  amount  was  placed  into  the 
bowl.  About  15-25  worms  were  given,  depending  on  the  skill  and  interest  level  of  each  bird.  The 
birds  that  caught  on  quickest  got  fewer  worms  than  those  who  were  slower  to  figure  it  out.  Peach 
fuzzies  were  also  in  the  shallow  pan  and  often  appeared  to  be  picked  up  accidentally,  as  they  would 
get  covered  by  the  worms.  Any  leftovers  were  placed  in  a ceramic  crock  with  more  meal  worms 
added,  then  placed  below  the  perch  so  they  could  see  the  movement  of  the  worms. 

Once  the  birds  caught  on  to  picking  up  food  on  their  own  from  the  crock,  the  crock  was  then  placed 
directly  into  a stand.  Crickets  were  initially  included  as  they  were  items  hand-fed;  however,  they 
were  never  eaten.  Although  some  birds  took  longer  than  others  to  figure  it  out,  the  transition  only 
took  a couple  of  weeks.  Their  diet  is  now  placed  in  a ceramic  crock  (as  plastic  feeders  get  flipped 
over).  The  crock  is  placed  in  a larger,  rectangular  stainless  steel  pan  lined  with  a mat  so  the  birds 
don’t  slip  when  landing.  This  pan  is  on  a stand  raised  off  the  ground.  It  is  important  to  leave  a clear 
flight  path  to  the  food  stand.  The  diet  is  offered  in  the  evening,  left  overnight  and  picked  up  in  the 
morning.  The  remaining  food  items  are  recorded.  Presently,  the  diet  per  bird  is  one  hopper,  six  peach 
fuzzies  and  mealworms.  The  amount  of  rodents  for  each  diet  varies.  Not  only  do  individuals  have 


Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5  165 


different  appetites,  but  throughout  the  year  appetites  fluctuate.  Amounts  are  adjusted  as  is  deemed 
necessary.  Today  all  of  our  Tawny  frogmouths  pan  feed.  With  this  new  feeding  protocol  in  place,  we 
hope  to  give  the  birds  a chance  to  raise  their  own  offspring. 

Introducing  Pairs 

Introducing  Tawny  frogmouths  is  much  less 
dramatic  than  many  other  species.  Initially  birds 
are  placed  side-by-side  in  howdy  cages.  We  used 
hanging  cages,  measuring  4ft.x4ft.x4ft.  [1.2m  x 
1.2m  X 1.2m]  suspended  from  the  ceiling.  The 
next  step  is  placing  them  in  a cage  together  with 
limited  perches  so  they  are  encouraged  to  be 
near  each  other.  Sitting  side  by  side  is  usually 
the  first  indication  of  pair  bonding.  One  pair  was 
introduced  by  this  method  and  after  two  months 
of  perching  on  opposite  sides  of  the  enclosure, 
it  was  decided  they  were  an  incompatible  pair. 

There  was  also  one  opportunity  to  house  the  male 
with  two  females.  This  worked  to  the  male’s  advantage  as  he  did  choose  one  female  for  his  mate. 
If  using  this  method,  monitor  the  birds  closely  and  remove  the  extra  female  once  the  male  makes 
his  choice  to  avoid  aggression.  Even  with  our  successes,  because  they  are  nocturnal,  frogmouth 
breeding  is  still  somewhat  mysterious  to  us.  A pair  is  put  together,  a nest  given  and  eggs  are  found. 
No  courtship  is  seen  and  no  bonding  behaviors  are  observed.  Occasionally  the  birds  will  be  witnessed 
adding  nesting  material  to  their  nest  site.  The  only  indication  we’ve  seen  that  a pair  has  perhaps 
bonded  prior  to  egg  laying  is  perching  in  close  proximity  to  each  other  and  vocalizing  back  and  forth. 
Both  sexes  vocalize,  but  the  females  are  often  more  vocal  than  the  males.  They  are  most  vocal  in  the 
early  morning,  evening,  and  before  breeding  season.  Birds  seem  to  be  quite  vocal  if  they  are  within 
sight  of  any  other  Tawny  frogmouths. 

Nesting 

Nests  given  are  constructed  with  a grapevine  wreath  cable  tied  to  a shade  cloth  bottom,  just  large 
enough  for  them  to  fit  in,  about  the  size  of  a salad  plate.  The  nests  are  secured  to  perches  or  in 
areas  that  the  birds  seem  to  occupy  frequently.  Multiple  nest  sites  are  offered,  as  they  may  be 


Feeding  Stand 


Photo  by  Liane  Berlepsch 


Nest  Constructed  by  Birds  Grapevine  Nest 

(Photo  by  Liane  Berlepsch)  (Photo  by  Liane  Berlepsch) 

particular  about  where  they  decide  to  nest.  Pairs  have  also  utilized  upright  palm  logs  that  were  in  the 
exhibit  as  decor.  Both  tall  and  short  logs,  with  either  a slight  depression  or  a rotted-out  interior,  have 
been  chosen  and  occupied  as  nests.  The  amount  of  nesting  material  used  seems  dependent  on  the 
individual.  One  aviary  had  only  one  potted  palm  plant  and  broken  and  stripped  leaves  were  found 
in  the  nest.  It  is  unknown  whether  the  female  builds,  but  males  definitely  do.  One  male,  who  was 


166  Animal  Keepers'  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5 


unpaired,  chose  an  empty  hollow  palm  log  as  his  perch  of  choice.  One  spring  he  made  it  into  a nest, 
filling  it  with  twigs  and  leaves,  and  occupied  it  comfortably  until  we  dislodged  it.  With  one  pair,  the 
male  dominated  the  nest  site,  often  sitting  on  it  days  prior  to  eggs  actually  being  laid.  It  was  even 
unknown  if  the  female  did  any  incubating,  as  the  male  was  on  the  nest  when  we  left  at  night  and 
upon  arrival  in  the  morning.  However,  with  one  of  the  newer  pairs,  both  male  and  female  were  seen 
randomly  on  the  nest  and  after  eggs  were  laid,  both  were  seen  on  the  nest  incubating. 


Incubation  and  Hand-rearing 

Tawny  frogmouths  have  laid  eggs  from  December  to  May.  They  usually  lay  two  eggs  per  clutch 
and  some  will  re-clutch  if  given  the  opportunity.  With  inexperienced  birds  there  is  often  hesitation 
leaving  valuable  eggs  with  them.  Eggs  are  pulled  from  the  nest  and  placed  into  an  incubator  and  a 
dummy  egg  is  left  for  the  birds  to  tend  to.  The  artificial  incubation  parameters  for  the  Orlando  climate 
are  99.5°F  [37.5°C]  for  the  dry  bulb  and  44-48%  relative  humidity.  If  a pair  proved  to  be  diligent  in 
incubating  the  dummy  eggs,  their  real  eggs  were  given  back  to  them,  and  any  future  eggs  left  with 
them.  They  will  only  be  put  in  an  incubator  if  absolutely  necessary;  for  example,  abandonment  of  an 
egg,  or  a pair  that  consistently  breaks  eggs.  Because  the  birds  have  yet  to  raise  chicks,  the  eggs  are 
left  in  the  nest  until  Day  25  (first  egg)  and  then  pulled  and  placed  into  an  incubator  until  hatching. 
This  is  three  days  prior  to  that  egg’s  predicted  hatch  date.  Once  externally  pipped  and  placed  into  a 
hatching  bowl  or  tray,  hatching  may  take  36-48  hours.  All  incubator-hatched  chicks  are  set  up  in  a 
brooder  to  be  hand-raised  by  an  aviculturist.  The  chick  is  placed  in  a small  bowl  appropriate  for  its 
size,  lined  with  a paper  towel  so  the  consistency 
of  the  feces  can  be  easily  monitored.  (We  use 
Wypall*  brand®  towels).  The  size  of  the  bowl  will 
grow  with  the  chick.  Temperature  of  the  brooder 
is  started  at  96°F  [35.5°C].  A specimen  cup,  with 
holes  punched  in  the  lid,  filled  with  a wet  sponge 

maintains  the  humidity.  If  the  chick  needs  a higher  -^Seriesi  i 

humidity,  a warmed,  damp  towel  can  be  placed  in 
the  brooder.  It  is  important  to  keep  the  temperature 
and  humidity  high  until  the  chick’s  first  defecation. 

A Tawny  chick  may  not  defecate  for  three  days, 
which  is  not  unusual.  Stimulation  may  be 
necessary  at  this  point,  but  not  beforehand.  Brooder 
temperature  should  be  dropped  as  necessary,  as  the 

chick  grows  feathers.  Allow  the  chick’s  comfort  level  to  determine  the  temperature. 


Tawny  Frogmouth  chick 


day  day  day  day  day  day  day  day  day  day  day  day  day  day 
2 8 15  22  29  36  42  49  56  64  66  67  69  75 
Age 


'H 


The  first  feeding  is  given  12  hours  after  hatching.  Some 
chicks  will  give  strong  feeding  responses  right  away,  while 
others  may  need  to  be  stimulated  with  a tap  on  the  side  of 
their  bill.  Tweezers  are  used  to  feed  the  chicks,  as  initial 
food  items  are  rather  small.  The  chick  is  fed  pinkie  viscera 
initially,  graduating  up  to  cut  pinkies,  and  eventually  whole 
pinkies  as  the  chick  ages.  The  chick  is  fed  until  satiated. 
However,  the  first  day,  or  until  it  defecates,  we  are  more 
conservative  with  the  amounts.  The  first  two  days  soak  the 
viscera  in  warm  lactated  Ringers  injection,  USP®  and  then 
just  warm  water  from  that  point  on.  Calcium  carbonate 
and  Vionate®  at  a 50:50  ratio,  are  sprinkled  on  food  items 
at  every  feeding.  Chicks  need  to  be  fed  with  a heat  source, 
as  they  chill  quickly  when  out  of  the  brooder.  Heat  lamps 
may  cause  retinal  damage  in  a nocturnal  bird,  thus  a 
heating  pad  or  ceramic  bulb  is  preferred.  To  measure  the 
small  amounts  fed,  obtain  the  weights  of  the  chick  before 
and  after  feeding.  Chicks  are  fed  five  times  per  day,  three 
and  a half  to  four  hours  apart.  At  approximately  two  weeks  of  age,  or  as  a chick  loses  interest  in  one 
or  more  feedings,  the  feedings  will  drop  to  four  per  day.  Again,  at  about  three  weeks  of  age,  the  chick 
should  be  down  to  three  feedings  per  day  and  comfortable  at  room  temperature. 


Tawny  Frogmouth  Chick 

(Photo  by  Sharon  Jarvis) 


Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5  167 


Aside  from  temperature,  humidity  and  feedings,  it  is  important  to  pay  attention  to  the  chicks’ 
legs.  The  bowl  bottom  should  be  lined  with  a substrate,  such  as  Enkamat®,  to  give  the  chicks’  feet 
something  to  grip  on  to.  This  needs  to  be  implemented  no  later  than  one  week,  as  splaying  of  legs  has 
occurred.  One  peculiarity  that  appears  in  some  frogmouth  chicks  is  a milky  coloration  to  their  eyes. 
This  is  normal  and  should  not  be  a cause  for  concern.  By  necessity  we  have  become  proficient  at 
hand-rearing,  however,  the  ultimate  goal  is  to  be  hands-off,  allowing  the  parents  the  opportunity  to 
care  for  their  young.  Now  that  all  the  frogmouths  are  self-feeding,  there  is  hope  that  future  chicks 
will  be  parent-reared.  Close  monitoring  of  the  chicks  will  take  place  and  they  will  be  pulled  for  hand- 
rearing or  supplemented,  if  necessary.  However,  the  breeding  pairs  are  very  attentive  to  their  eggs, 
so  we  are  hopeful  that  they  will  be  successful  in  rearing  their  own  young. 

Reference 

Myers,  Mark.  2009.  Long-term  Propagation  Program.  Plume  Award. 

AZA  Avian  Scientific  Advisory  Group. 


from  four  cJiicks  from  2007-08  ages  1 to  15  days. 


Age 

Average  weight  of  ciiicksCg) 

Average  amount  fed  per  day 

I 

15 

0.73* 

2 

15 

1.13 

3 

20 

1.15 

4 

22 

1.49 

5 

24 

1.64 

6 

27 

2.2 

7 

32 

1-8** 

8 

39 

8-Feb 

9 

48 

9- Apr 

10 

58 

6- Feb 

1 I 

66 

1 1-Jan 

12 

81 

8-Apr 

13 

90 

1 0-Mar 

14 

100 

10-Apr 

15 

1 14 

10- Mar 

*grams  of  pinkie  viscera 

**at  this  point  pinkies  canbe  fed  with  heads,  tails,  feet  cut  off  and  into  thirds  or  halves, 
depending  on  the  size  of  the  pinkie  and  chick.  These  numbers  are  how  many  pinkies  are 
used  and  ranged  of  how  many  are  fed  at  each  feeding. 


Products  mentioned  in  the  text: 


Vionate® 

Email:  pets@gimbom.com 

Gimbom  Pet  Specialties,  EEC 

4280  N.E.  Expressway,  Atlanta,  GA  30340 

Tel:  770-454-3200  Fax:  770-454-3214 

Fax  toll  free:  800-745-0544 


Enkamat® 

www.colbond-usa.com 
Colbond,  Inc. 

PO  Box  1057,  Enka,  NC  28728 
Tel:  1-800-365-7391 
Fax:  828-665-5009 


Wypall*brand® 

www.kcprofessional.com 

Email:  kcpinfo@kcc.con 

®/*Trademarks  of  Kimberly-Clark  Worldwide,  Inc. 

Kimberly-Clark  Global  Sales,  EEC 

Roswell,  GA  30076 

Tel:  888-346-4652 


Eactated  Ringer’s  Injection,  USP® 

www.hospira.com 

275  North  Field  Drive 

Hospira,  Inc.,  Eake  Forest,  IE  60045 

Tel:  877-946-7747 


168  Animal  Keepers'  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5 


Incubation  and  Hand-rearing  of  a Greater  Rhea 
(Rhea  americana)  at  the  Great  Plains  Zoo 

By  Cassandra  Sampson,  Area  Supervisor  and  Hans  Jorgensen,  Zookeeper 
Great  Plains  Zoo,  Sioux  Falls,  SD 


Introduction 

The  Great  Plains  Zoo  is  a 45-acre  zoological  facility  in  Sioux  Falls,  SD  that  houses  over  1 50  species  of 
animals.  Among  the  zoo’s  52  species  of  birds,  three  ratite  species  are  represented,  including  Ostrich 
(Struthio  camelus),  Emu  {Dromaius  novae hollandiae),  and  Greater  Rhea  {Rhea  Americana). 

The  Greater  rhea  is  one  of  five  species  of  flightless  birds  known  as  ratites.  Native  to  the  pampas 
and  sparse  woodlands  of  eastern  South  America,  these  birds  are  considered  nearly  threatened  by  the 
lUCN.  Rheas  are  relatively  common  in  their  home  range,  but  their  numbers  appear  to  be  declining 
in  the  wild.  According  to  ISIS  (International  Species  Information  System)  data,  there  are  currently 
273  Greater  rhea  being  held  in  North  American  ISIS  institutions,  with  only  15  birds  being  hatched 
out  in  2008. 

The  Great  Plains  Zoo  has  had  a group  of  rheas  on  display  since  2003.  The  original  group  of  birds  was 
all  related;  therefore  any  eggs  laid  were  discarded.  In  2008,  after  shipping  out  one  of  our  males  and 
losing  our  second  to  old  age,  the  decision  was  made  to  increase  our  flock  size  and  breeding  potential 
by  hatching  out  eggs  brought  in  from  another  facility. 

Like  other  ratites,  the  males  of  the  species  are  the  ones  to  incubate  the  eggs  and  care  for  the  chicks 
after  hatching.  Since  our  current  group  was  comprised  of  only  females,  any  chicks  we  hatched  out 
would  have  to  be  hand-reared. 

After  searching  for  institutions  that  were  breeding  rheas  to  check  into  the  availability  of  eggs,  it 
became  apparent  that  the  population  of  rheas  in  zoological  facilities  in  the  United  States  had  been 
aging  in  the  last  decade  and  that  few  birds  were  being  added  to  the  population. 

In  April  of  2008,  we  received  three  different  shipments  of  eggs,  for  a total  of  seven,  from  Lion 
Country  Safari  in  West  Palm  Beach,  FL.  The  eggs  were  shipped  via  overnight  mail  by  the  United 
States  Postal  Service  in  foam  laden  containers  in  an  attempt  to  limit  unnecessary  shifting  during 
shipment. 

Incubation 

Upon  arrival,  the  eggs  were  placed  in  our  Humidaire®  incubator.  Each  egg  was  weighed  daily  and 
candled  occasionally  to  check  for  viability.  Six  of  the  seven  eggs  were  eventually  pulled  from  the 
incubator  and  discarded  based  on  candling  observations.  The  remaining  egg  hatched.  It  was  laid  on 
12  April  2008  and  was  placed  in  our  incubator  on  15  April.  The  incubator  was  set  to  79°F  [26.1°C] 
on  the  wet  bulb  with  45%  relative  humidity  to  achieve  a 15%  weight  loss  through  incubation.  The 
incubation  period  for  Greater  rheas  is  36-40  days.  Our  chick  hatched  out  on  20  May,  after  36  days  of 
incubation.  The  egg  actually  lost  18%  of  its  weight  over  the  course  of  incubation. 

Hand-rearing 

The  chick  was  kept  in  our  veterinary  facility  in  a small  stainless  steel  cage  with  supplemental  heat  to 
begin  with,  and  was  moved  to  a larger  pen  in  the  same  building  when  size  necessitated  it.  Impaction 
can  be  a major  concern  with  young  ratites,  as  they  are  inclined  to  eat  any  small  object  that  they 
see.  Keeping  our  chick  inside  allowed  us  to  control  the  temperature,  as  well  as  the  substrate,  thus 
eliminating  impaction  concerns. 


Animal  Keepers’ Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5  169 


The  food  we  decided  to  offer  the  chick  was 
Mazuri®  brand  Ratite  Starter,  but  had  trouble 
receiving  it  in  time,  so  we  had  to  start  with  a 
less  than  ideal  waterfowl  starter.  With  parent- 
reared  chicks,  you  usually  worry  about  giving 
too  much  food  right  away,  because  of  impaction 
concerns,  but  with  our  lone  chick,  getting  it  to  < 
eat  was  the  challenge,  so  no  food  restrictions 
were  put  in  place.  We  housed  the  chick  on 
newspaper,  and  scattered  the  food  on  the  paper 
so  that  when  it  walked  around,  the  food  would 
“jump”,  as  well  as  offering  a dish  of  food.  We 
also  tried  sprinkling  chopped  greens  in  the 
food  and  water,  and  offering  mealworms.  A 
chicken  was  also  placed  in  its  pen  for  awhile 
in  the  hopes  that  it  would  show  the  chick  how 
to  eat,  but  to  no  avail.  Several  times  a day,  we 
would  take  the  chick  out,  put  it  on  the  floor,  and 
try  to  show  it  how  to  eat  by  pecking  at  food 
scattered  on  the  floor  with  our  finger  or  shaking 
the  food  on  a piece  of  newspaper.  This  seemed 
to  be  the  most  successful  way  of  getting  our  i 
chick  to  eat  in  the  first  two  weeks.  It  wasn’t  ; 
until  we  received  the  ratite  starter  on  Day  14 
that  it  really  started  to  eat  well  on  its  own.  We  weighed  the  chick  daily,  and  after  Day  15  its  weight 
consistently  increased  (See  Table  1 and  Figure  1).  We  started  adding  granite  grit  to  the  diet  on  Day 
8 at  3%  by  weight  to  condition  the  ventriculus  for  forage. 

It  was  very  clear  that  the  chick  was  imprinted  on  people  and  did  not  like  being  left  alone  in  the  cage. 

It  would  vocalize  almost  constantly  when  left  alone.  By  June,  the  chick  was  trying  to  get  out  of  its  j 
cage,  and  a towel  had  to  be  hung  on  the  cage  door  to  stop  it  from  rubbing  its  beak.  Unfortunately,  an 
abrasion  on  its  beak  did  develop,  but  applying  Vetropolycin  ointment  cleared  it  up.  ! 

We  started  taking  it  outside  for  walks  in  the  grass  on  6 June.  It  would  follow  the  keeper  outside  and 
eat  grass.  We  wanted  to  gradually  introduce  forage  into  its  diet.  By  1 3 June,  we  decided  to  make  a 
temporary  outdoor  pen  that  the  chick  could  spend  a few  hours  a day  in  if  the  weather  was  nice.  We 
constructed  a small  make-shift  pen  approximately  4’x4’x4’  [1.21mx  1.21m  x 1.21m]  out  of  chicken 
wire  wrapped  around  T-posts  and  placed  it  under  a willow  tree  on  a grassy  lawn  just  outside  our  i 
veterinary  facility.  Being  outside  gave  it  the  opportunity  to  get  natural  sunlight  as  well  as  eat  grass.  ; 
It  eventually  started  rubbing  its  beak  against  the  chicken  wire  trying  to  get  out,  so  we  hung  sheets  on  j 
the  inside  of  the  chicken  wire  to  eliminate  the  rubbing.  ' 

Also  in  June  we  sent  a blood  sample  to  the  Brookfield  Zoo  for  sex  determination.  The  test  showed  ii 
that  we  had  a female  chick.  Since  we  really  needed  a male  to  form  a breeding  group,  we  decided  || 
to  bring  in  2.3  young  parent-raised  rheas  from  Omaha’s  Henry  Doorly  Zoo.  Upon  their  arrival  in  | 
August,  our  chick  was  introduced  to  the  group  in  quarantine.  All  six  chicks  were  around  two  months  j 
old  at  this  time,  and  were  switched  from  Mazuri®  Ratite  Starter  to  Mazuri®  Ratite  Maintenance.  il 

In  September,  the  young  birds  were  introduced  to  our  three  adult  females  on  exhibit.  Our  hand-  |j 
reared  chick  did  tend  to  stay  off  by  herself  for  the  first  couple  of  months,  and  every  time  we  entered  |; 
the  exhibit  she  would  come  right  over,  but  in  time  she  started  to  associate  herself  more  and  more  with  , 
the  other  birds.  ii 


Greater  Rhea  Chick 

(Photo  provided  by  the  authors) 


170  Animal  Keepers'  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5 


Table  1: 


Animal  Keepers’ Forum,  VoL  37  No.  4/5  171 


Figure  1: 


% Chick  weight  change 


Note:  No  data  available  for  days  20  and  25 

' ! 

V I 

Conclusion  ' 

Hand-rearing  of  our  Greater  rhea  proved  to  be  a fun  learning  experience,  with  a few  challenges  along  f 
the  way.  Getting  the  chick  to  start  eating  was  our  biggest  challenge,  and  a lot  of  time  was  invested 
in  showing  it  how  to  eat  in  the  first  two  weeks.  Other  challenges  were  faced  with  a little  creativity,  j 
such  as  hanging  sheets  on  the  caging  to  eliminate  the  chick’s  desire  to  get  out.  It  was  very  rewarding  h 
to  see  our  chick  from  hatching  all  the  way  to  integration  into  a group  of  conspecifics.  Our  experience 
helped  us  to  develop  a road  map  for  future  hand-rearing  of  ratites.  | 

Acknowledgements  | 

We  would  like  to  thank  the  Ratite  TAG  Chair,  Sara  Hallager,  from  the  National  Zoo  for  her  guidance  v 
and  willingness  to  share  information,  as  well  as  the  staff  of  Lion  Country  Safari  for  taking  the  time  ;iv| 
to  collect  and  ship  the  eggs.  The  entire  staff  of  the  Great  Plains  Zoo  also  deserves  credit  for  their  0 
involvement  in  the  successful  hand-rearing  of  our  chick. 


172  Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5 


First  Time  Breeding  of  Captive  Sulawesi  Knobbed  Hornbills 
(Aceros  cassidix)  at  St.  Augustine  Alligator  Farm  Zoological  Park 

By  Stephanie  Krueger,  Senior  Bird  and  Mammal  Keeper 

St.  Augustine  Alligator  Farm  Zoological  Park,  St.  Augustine,  FL 


The  St.  Augustine  Alligator  Farm  Zoological  Park  acquired  a female  Sulawesi  knobbed  hombill 
{Aceros  cassidix)  in  late  2008,  and  after  successfully  introducing  her  to  the  resident  male,  the  park 
began  setting  up  an  on-exhibit  breeding  situation  for  the  2009  season.  Several  steps  were  taken 
to  encourage  breeding,  including:  research  of  species  courtship  and  breeding  requirements,  diet 
alternations  to  condition  the  birds  for  breeding,  and  supplying  an 


Because  this  species  had  never  been  bred  at  the  zoo  before, 
additional  information  was  gathered  about  how  and  when  these 
birds  breed.  Wild  Knobbed  hornbills  had  been  observed  breeding 
during  the  mid  to  late  summer  in  their  native  range  of  Sulawasi 
(Kinnarird,  1999,  p.61),  but  the  Florida  climate  of  St.  Augustine 
meant  that  similar  weather  conditions  of  temperature  and  rain 
occurred  earlier  in  the  year,  around  February  or  March.  So  as  to 
not  miss  the  ideal  breeding  time,  it  was  decided  to  set  up  the  birds 
at  the  very  beginning  of  2009. 


Courtship  behavior  and  nesting  requirements  were  the  next 
two  objectives  explored.  Using  behavioral  observation  (l.n.i 
provided  by  EAZA  Hombill  Management  and  Husbandly 
Guidelines,  staff  was  able  to  conduct  casual  observation  of  ihc 
pair  (Galama,  W.,  King,  C.,  Brouwer,  K.,  2002,  pgs.  59-61).  The 
observed  behaviors  included  allo-preening,  allo-feeding,  quiet 
vocalizations  to  one  another,  and  “shoving”  or  “pushing”  of  the 
male  by  the  female  when  perched  side  by  side.  These  behaviors 
were  observed  immediately  upon  introductions,  and  suggested  that  the  birds  had  bonded  and  would 
attempt  to  breed. 


Nest  Hole  - Mudding  behavior 
was  observed  by  both  the 
male  and  the  female  hornbills 

(Photo  by  Gen  Anderson) 


The  male,  Kong,  stands  watch  over  the  female,  Fay, 
who  is  putting  the  last  touches  on  the  mudded  nest 

entryway.  (Photo  by  Gen  Anderson) 


Next,  alterations  were  made  to  meet  breeding 
requirements.  Sulawesi  knobbed  hornbills, 
like  other  hornbills,  are  cavity  nesters.  Females 
will  seal  themselves  into  the  selected  cavity 
with  mud,  feces,  and  food.  Only  after  the 
female  is  sealed  in  will  she  lay  and  incubate 
eggs,  and  will  even  spend  several  weeks  after 
the  eggs  have  hatched  in  the  cavity  feeding 
and  caring  for  the  chicks.  During  the  females’ 
seclusion,  she  is  entirely  dependent  on  the 
male  for  food,  so  a quality  diet  and  a strong 
pair  bond  are  required  for  breeding  success. 
To  prepare  for  this,  hopper  mice  were  added 
to  the  previous  diet  for  additional  protein 
and  as  a gift-giving  opportunity  for  the  male 


Animal  Keepers'  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5  173 


at  the  beginning  of  January  2009.  Providing  valuable 
food  items  for  gift  giving  helps  establish  the  necessary 
pair  bond.  Also,  because  of  the  nest  sealing  behavior 
of  these  birds,  their  diet  was  made  stickier  with  the 
addition  of  ripe  bananas  and  soaked  softbill  pellet.  A 
small  bowl  of  wet  clay  was  also  offered  on  exhibit  for 
sealing  purposes  (Galama,  W.,  King,  C.,  Brouwer,  K., 
2002,  p.  49). 

The  final  step  was  the  addition  of  a suitable  nesting 
cavity.  Several  options  suggested  by  the  EAZA 
Hombill  Management  and  Husbandry  Guidelines  were 
explored,  including  cement  cylinders,  modified  box 
style  nest  cavities,  and  barrels  or  wine  casks.  The  final 
decision  was  to  use  a modified  wine  cask  as  the  nesting 
cavity.  Compared  to  the  other  options,  the  wine  barrel 
was  easier  to  modify,  the  appropriate  size  for  the  exhibit 
space,  and  readily  available  since  a suitable  cask  was 
already  on  property.  The  barrel  used  was  a wine  barrel 
measuring  55cm  [l.Sft.]  in  diameter  and  86cm  [2.8ft.] 
tall.  An  oblong  entry  hole,  dimensions  25cm  long  by 
14cm  [9.8in.  x 5.5in.]  across,  was  cut  into  the  barrel 
with  a jigsaw  55cm  up  from  the  bottom  of  the  barrel.  The  boards  of  the  barrel  were  2cm  [.78in.] 
thick,  so  the  edges  of  the  opening  were  roughed  up  with  a hand  saw  to  help  the  hombills  apply  sealing 
material.  A small  observation  door  measuring  20cm  x 20cm  [7.87in.  x 7.87in.]  was  cut  into  the  back 
of  the  barrel  for  keeper  observation  purposes.  The  barrel  was  filled  2/3  full  with  dry  pine  shavings 
and  was  installed  on  a ledge  on-exhibit  approximately  2.5m  [8.2ft.]  off  the  ground  with  a level 
plywood  roof  secured  on  top  of  the  barrel  to  shelter  it 
from  the  elements.  Lastly,  a perch  was  added  next  to 
the  barrel’s  entrance  to  facilitate  entry  into  the  nest  and 
feeding  of  the  female  by  the  male. 

After  the  barrel  was  placed  in  the  exhibit,  funding  was 
obtained  for  an  infra-red  camera  which  was  mounted  on 
the  barrel  roof,  allowing  staff  an  overhead  view  of  the 
inside  of  the  nest  barrel  available  for  viewing  24  hours 
a day.  This  camera  was  inexpensive  (approximately 
$100)  and  easily  installed  because  it  was  added  as 
an  additional  feed  line  on  the  pre-existing  security 
camera  network.  This  camera  proved  valuable  as  it 
allowed  staff  to  monitor  several  aspects  of  the  females’ 
seclusion,  including:  the  females’  condition  in  the  nest 
barrel,  how  much,  how  often,  and  of  what  variety  the 
male  was  feeding  the  female,  when  an  egg  was  laid, 
when  the  egg  hatched,  and  the  progress  of  the  resulting 
chick.  The  frequency  of  feedings  for  the  female  was 
an  initial  concern  because  this  was  an  inexperienced 
first-time  pair.  Also,  there  can  never  be  too  much 


After  several  long  months,  the  female 
emerges  from  the  nest  barrel,  but  frequently 
returns  to  check  on  the  chick  still  inside. 


(Photo  by  Gen  Anderson) 


Mudding  behavior  was  observed  over 
several  weeks.  However,  in  a single  week, 
the  female  secluded  herself  in  the  nest 
barrel  and  successfully  mudded  up  the 
entry  hole  on  her  own.  (Photo  by  Gen  Anderson) 


174  Animal  Keepers' Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5 


information  gathered  on  the  actions  and  success 
of  a breeding  pair  of  secretive  birds. 


Makassar  - The  first  successfully  bred  Knobbed 
Hombill  at  the  St.  Augustine  Alligator  Farm 

Zoological  Park.  {Photo  by  Greg  Lepera) 


Overall  this  project  was  a success  resulting  in  the 
hatch  and  fledge  of  a single  Sulawesi  knobbed 
hombill  chick.  From  this  breeding  experience, 
several  observations  can  be  made.  First,  the  diet 
alterations  seemed  to  increase  bonding  behavior 
between  the  birds,  and  the  addition  of  sticky 
material  stimulated  sealing  behavior  in  both  the 
male  and  the  female.  For  the  barrel,  the  size  of 
the  cask,  the  nesting  material  within,  and  the 
location  of  the  barrel  were  adequate.  However,  a 
more  substantial  and  angled  roof  to  better  shelter 
the  barrel  from  rain  should  be  added,  and  a lower 
entry  hole  should  be  cut  so  the  female  and  young 
chick  have  easier  access  to  the  male  outside. 

Lastly,  the  infra-red  camera  proved  an  invaluable 
tool  for  data  collecting,  but  will  also  need  some 
modifications.  The  camera  was  installed  directly 
into  the  roof  of  the  barrel  and  was  sheltered  by 
an  arrangement  of  plywood  and  heavy  rocks,  and 
after  the  barrel  was  put  on  the  exhibit  the  female 
became  nervous  when  she  visited  the  nest  barrel 
and  spent  several  weeks  picking  at  the  camera 
lens  in  the  ceiling.  Because  of  this,  the  camera 
became  damaged  towards  the  end  of  the  season. 
A more  substantial  housing  and  installation  prior 


to  the  breeding  season  should  allow  the  birds 
more  time  to  adjust  to  its  presence  in  the  barrel 
and  should  solve  this  problem.  Also,  the  camera 
was  part  of  the  security  system  and  meant 
keepers  only  had  limited  access  for  viewing 
purposes.  Hopefully,  an  additional  or  separate 
server  may  allow  more  “open”  viewing  access 
for  keepers  in  the  future. 

References 

Kinnaird,  M.F.,  O’Brien,  T.  G.  1999.  Breeding 
ecology  of  the  Sulawesi  Red-Knobbed  Hombill 
(Aceros  cassidix).  Ibis.  Volume  141,  Issue  1: 
60-69.  British  Ornithologists’  Union, 

Galama,  W.,  King,  C.,  Brouwer,  K..  2002. 
EAZA  Hombill  Management  and  Husbandry 
Guidelines.  1st  Edition.  The  EAZA  Hombill 
TAG,  Amsterdam,  The  Netherlands 


Have  you  registered  for  the 
Members  Only  section  on  the 
AAZK  Website?  If  not,  you 
are  missing  out  on  lots  of  great 
resources  and  information. 
Visit  www.aazk.org 


^ -THE 

GOURMET 

RODENT, 

INC." 

^ aiSiy 


RATS  AND  MICE 


Bill  & Marcia  Brant 

12921  SWlstRd.,Ste  107, 
PBM  #434 
Jonesville,  FL  32669 

(352)  472-9189 
Fax:  (352)  472-9192 
e-mail:  GrmtRodent@aol.com 


Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5  175 


Artificially  Inseminating  White-naped  Cranes 


(II 


By  Chris  Crowe,  Bird  Keeper 

National  Zoo  ’5  Conservation  and  Research  Center 

1500  Remount  Road,  Front  Royal,  VA  22630 


Introduction 

With  their  loud  bugling  calls,  stately  gait,  and  tall  posture  cranes  are  among  the  most  majestic  birds 
in  the  world.  They  are  also  one  of  the  most  endangered  bird  families  with  1 1 out  of  1 5 crane  species 
endangered  worldwide.  The  primary  threats  to  their  survival  are  habitat  loss,  habitat  degradation,  and 
human  disturbance  as  the  wetlands  they  nest  and  feed  in  are  altered  or  destroyed  by  humans.  There 
are  less  than  5000  White-naped  cranes  (Grus  vipio)  left  in  the  wilds  of  China,  Mongolia,  Russia, 
Japan,  and  North  and  South  Korea.  The  endangered  White-naped  cranes  cared  for  at  the  National 
Zoo’s  Conservation  and  Research  Center  (NZP-CRC)  are  part  of  an  international  captive  breeding 
program  managed  under  the  auspices  of  a Species  Survival  Plan  (SSP®)  with  the  goal  of  maintaining 
a security  population  to  save  the  species  from  extinction  in  the  event  that  the  wild  population  is 
extirpated.  Breeding  is  managed  to  ensure  that  the  population  is  demographically  stable,  genetically 
diverse,  healthy,  and  self-sustaining.  Occasionally  behavioral  or  physical  limitations  prevent  certain 
cranes  from  breeding  naturally,  requiring  the  use  of  artificial  insemination  (A. I.).  At  NZP-CRC 
we  have  come  to  specialize  in  using  A.I.  to  produce  offspring  from  cranes  that  are  unable  to  breed 
naturally.  This  encompasses  cranes  other  zoos  were  unable  to  breed,  including  the  most  genetically 
valuable  White-naped  cranes  in  the  SSP®.  Not  only  have  we  been  able  to  produce  chicks  from 
these  birds  through  A. I.,  but  we  were  also  able  to  train  them  to  accept  artificial  insemination  without 
physical  restraint. 

Artificial  Insemination 

The  technique  of  A.I.  requires  the  handling  and  restraint  of  cranes  for  both  semen  collection  from  the 
male  and  artificial  insemination  of  the  female.  Every  effort  is  made  to  reduce  the  chance  of  injury 
and  stress  to  both  birds  and  their  keepers  during  the  A.I.  procedure.  The  main  way  to  reduce  stress  for 
a crane  is  to  always  conduct  the  A.I.  training  and  eventual  procedure  in  the  same  way  with  the  same 
people.  Training  is  conducted  in  exactly  the  same  way  as  the  eventual  procedure  so  that  once  the 
cranes  are  responding  properly  the  change  to  actual  semen  collection  and  A.I.  will  be  as  seamless  as 
possible.  The  cranes  will  never  enjoy  being  captured  and  restrained,  but  as  they  become  familiar  with 
the  process  their  stress  level  will  decrease.  Their  stress  and  risk  of  injury  declines  as  they  realize  they 
will  not  be  harmed  and  the  efficiency  and  quality  of  the  procedures  increases  once  the  birds  know 

what  to  expect.  The  A.I.  technique  we  use 
involves  manually  massaging  the  male  crane  to 
stimulate  him  to  produce  the  semen  and  soon 
afterwards  manually  massaging  the  female  so 
she  will  accept  artificial  insemination.  While 
the  cranes  are  held  against  their  will,  they  are 
gently  stimulated  to  respond  reproductively 
and  cooperate  with  the  procedure. 

Abigail  the  White-naped  Crane 
Abigail  was  hatched  overseas  and  arrived  at 
NZP-CRC  in  1 998  at  the  age  of  1 6 having  never 
produced  any  offspring.  She  had  been  paired 
with  a male  while  at  her  previous  institution 
but  despite  their  strong  bond,  impressive 
nests,  and  faithful  egg  incubation,  they  never 
hatched  any  chicks.  Soon  after  Abigail’s  arrival  we  noticed  that  she  would  occasionally  favor  her 


Abigail  with  Amanda’s  chick. 


i 

g 

I 


176  Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5 


left  leg.  The  medical  records  from  her  time  at  other  institutions  noted  occasional  left  leg  lameness  all 
throughout  her  previous  16  years.  We  brought  Abigail  in  for  x-rays  to  diagnose  her  injury  and  develop 
possible  treatment.  The  radiographs  revealed  both  the  cause  of  her  limping  and  the  reason  she  had 
never  produced  any  chicks.  Abigail’s  left  femur  was  missing  its  femoral  head  which  prevented  the 
femur  from  articulating  with  the  pelvic  girdle.  Her  dislocated  femur  extended  beyond  the  pelvis  and 
towards  her  lower  back.  The  proximal  end  of  the  femur  could  actually  be  felt  protruding  below  the 
feathers,  skin,  and  muscles  on  the  crane’s  back.  The  obvious  result  was  a decrease  in  muscle  mass 
and  range  of  motion  for  her  left  leg.  This  caused  Abigail  to  limp  periodically  and  prevented  her  from 
being  able  to  support  a male’s  weight  during  copulation.  Given  her  long  history  of  favoring  her  left 
leg,  we  suspected  Abigail’s  condition  was  likely  due  to  a traumatic  injury  that  occurred  when  she  was 
a chick  and  was  somehow  previously  undetected.  She  somehow  adapted  to  the  injury  and  learned 
to  function  normally  except  for  occasionally  limping  and  being  unable  to  breed  naturally.  At  the 
time  of  the  diagnosis,  her  long-term  prognosis  was  poor.  Over  time  her  bouts  of  limping  became 
less  common  and  severe.  Because  of  her  physical  limitations  to  breeding,  Abigail  became  a prime 
candidate  for  A.I.  Had  we  considered  her  condition  to  be  genetic  we  would  not  have  allowed  her  to 
breed.  She  was  paired  with  an  SSP®  recommended  male  that  was  10  years  her  junior  and  almost  a 
foot  taller  than  the  diminutive  Abigail.  Despite  their  age  disparity  and  height  differential,  Abigail 
and  “Ray”  got  along  well. 

the  White-naped  Crane 

To  collect  semen  from  male  cranes  requires  catching  and  restraining  them.  Once  caught,  a male  is 
held  by  one  handler  with  its  body  between  the  handlers  legs  and  the  crane’s  head  (and  dangerous 
beak)  behind  the  handler  and  facing  away  from  the  handler’s  back.  The  bird  is  primarily  restrained 
by  the  handler  applying  pressure  from  their  legs  around  the  cranes  body  while  massaging  the  male’s 
thighs.  To  facilitate  the  male  producing  a semen  sample,  the  crane  must  be  restrained  gently  enough 

to  allow  the  bird  to  stand  upright  but  still  firm 
enough  to  prevent  escape.  A second  person 
kneels  down  and  manually  massages  the  male’s 
lower  abdomen,  lower  back,  top  of  the  tail,  and 
cloacal  area.  When  stimulated  properly  the 
male’s  cloaca  will  begin  to  pulsate.  When  this 
happens  the  second  person  gently  everts  the 
cloaca  while  holding  a shot  glass  right  up  to 
the  cloacal  vent  to  collect  the  semen  droplet. 
Once  the  crane  is  carefully  released  and  the 
keepers  have  vacated  the  enclosure,  a syringe 
with  semen  extender  is  used  to  draw  up  the 
sample  and  the  sample  is  quickly  transported 
for  the  A.I.  of  the  female.  After  the  A. I., 
the  small  amount  of  semen  remaining  in  the 
syringe  is  looked  at  under  a microscope  to  check  for  the  presence  of  sperm  and  determine  its  viability, 
concentration  and  mobility. 

Once  captured  and  restrained,  Ray  always  produced  quality  semen  samples  and  was  very  cooperative 
during  the  semen  collection  procedure.  The  only  problem  in  collecting  semen  from  him  occurred 
during  the  initial  capture  and  later  release.  We  typically  capture  our  cranes  by  methodically  herding 
them  into  a comer  of  their  enclosure  and  safely  but  quickly  capturing  them  once  they  turn  their  back 
to  us  while  facing  the  comer.  Cranes  respond  to  threats  by  pecking  with  their  beak  (usually  toward 
the  eyes)  and  jumping  and  kicking  their  legs  to  rake  the  perceived  threat  with  their  claws.  So  we  wait 
until  their  back  is  turned  to  protect  ourselves  from  their  pointed  bill  and  sharp  claws.  However,  Ray 
is  our  only  crane  that  refuses  to  turn  his  back  on  us  when  herded  into  a comer.  The  five-foot  tall  male 
will  turn  and  directly  face  us  whenever  we  approach  closely.  This  made  him  an  excellent  defender 
of  nests,  eggs,  and  chicks  but  a bit  of  a challenge  for  semen  collection.  We  learned  to  wear  safety 


Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5  111 


goggles  and  adapt  our  capture  approach.  By  extending  a broom  towards  Ray,  a keeper  can  entice  the 
male  to  peck  at  the  broom  and  then  quickly  grab  the  bird’s  neck  and  corral  the  body.  Releasing  Ray 
is  just  as  much  of  a challenge  because  he  will  turn  around  and  attack  as  soon  as  we  let  go  of  him. 

Remember  that  during  the  semen  collection,  the  male  crane  is  restrained  by  holding  its  body  between 
the  handler’s  legs  and  holding  the  bird’s  thighs  by  hand.  Releasing  the  bird  involves  removing  our 
hands  from  his  thighs,  relieving  our  leg’s  pressure  on  his  body  and  allowing  the  male  to  walk  under 
our  legs  and  out  the  other  side.  Initially  upon  such  release,  the  male  is  facing  away  from  the  handler’s 
back  but  is  in  very  close  proximity  to  the  handler  who  has  limited  visibility  to  the  crane  behind  him. 
Ray  would  immediately  turn  around  to  face  us  and  often  attack.  We  learned  to  safely  release  Ray  by 
giving  him  a gentle  push  as  his  handler  lets  go  and  extending  a broom  towards  him  to  draw  his  aim 
away  from  our  face  if  he  decides  to  attack.  Since  employing  a broom,  Ray  typically  lets  us  off  with 
just  a threat  display  after  his  release. 

Breeding  Abigail  and  Ray 

The  semen  collections  and  artificial  inseminations  went  smoothly  and  effectively  once  Abigail  and 
Ray  were  trained.  Because  neither  bird  had  ever  raised  chicks  before  we  gave  their  first  chick  to 
an  experienced  pair  to  foster  raise  as  their  own.  We  didn’t  want  to  risk  leaving  such  a genetically 
valuable  chick  with  first-time  parents.  In  exchange,  Abigail  and  Ray  were  given  the  fertile  egg  from 
a common  Florida  Sandhill  crane  pair  to  hatch,  incubate,  and  raise  as  their  own.  They  turned  out  to 
be  excellent  parents  and  in  the  future  were  allowed  to  raise  their  own  chicks  and  eventually  foster- 
raise other  chicks. 

Twice  during  their  pairing  Ray  has  become  aggressive  towards  Abigail  during  the  start  of  breeding 
season.  The  first  incident  occurred  early  on  in  their  pairing  and  was  likely  due  to  the  female’s 
inability  to  allow  the  male  to  copulate.  The  second  incident  happened  two  years  ago  when  Abigail 
was  egg  bound.  The  male  probably  responded  harshly  to  behavior  by  the  female  that  he  could  not 
understand.  In  both  cases,  the  male  was  temporarily  separated  into  an  adjacent  pen.  Once  Abigail 
laid  her  first  egg,  Ray  was  allowed  back  into  her  pen  and  predictably  focused  his  attention  and  energy 
on  keeping  perceived  predators  (i.e.  keepers,  passing  cars,  etc)  away  from  his  precious  nest.  As  a 
precaution  we  separate  the  pair  every  spring  near  the  average  lay  date  for  the  female’s  first  egg  of  the 
year.  While  separated,  Abigail  always  lays  her  first  egg  in  a nest  alongside  the  common  fence  line 
with  Ray  and  once  she  has  laid  her  egg  we  let  him  rejoin  her. 

Through  A. I.  we  produced  one  chick  from  the  pair  in  2002  and  a second  chick  in  2003.  Both  chicks 
were  hatched  healthy  and  raised  wonderfully  by  their  parents.  In  addition,  Abigail  and  Ray  provided 
four  fertile  eggs  to  an  effort  to  reintroduce  White-naped  cranes  to  the  wild.  In  cooperation  with 
other  accredited  zoos,  her  fertile  eggs  were  sent  to  Russia  to  be  incubated,  hatched,  raised,  and 
released  to  the  wilds  of  Muraviovka  Wildlife  Reserve.  The  two  are  such  model  parents  that  they 
have  foster-raised  five  chicks  from  eggs  produced  by  other  White-naped  cranes.  In  2009,  Abigail 
and  Ray  foster-raised  their  granddaughter,  a female  chick  produced  from  A.I.  involving  their  male 
chick  from  2002. 

Artificial  Insemination  without  Restraint 

Artificially  inseminating  cranes  normally  requires  a crane  to  be  stationary  while  being  stimulated  and 
inseminated.  This  entails  capturing  the  crane  and  manually  restraining  it  during  the  entire  procedure. 
While  this  type  of  A.I.  is  very  effective  in  producing  the  desired  results  of  representing  the  bird’s 
genetics,  the  handling  is  stressful  for  the  cranes.  Their  stress  level  complicates  the  A.i.’s  efficiency 
by  often  causing  them  to  defecate  during  the  procedure.  There  is  no  alternative  to  restraining  the 
crane  when  dealing  with  cranes  requiring  A.I.  due  to  physical  limitations.  But  for  female  cranes  with 
the  behavioral  problem  of  having  imprinted  on  people  we  have  developed  a preferred  alternative. 
By  training  the  imprinted  cranes  to  accept  A.I.  without  restraint  we  used  each  bird’s  problematic 
behavior  to  both  the  bird’s  and  the  species’  advantage. 


178  Animal  Keepers'  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5 


^^alnuf^  the  White-naped  Crane 

Walnut  the  White-naped  crane  arrived  at  NZP-CRC  in  2004.  She  had  been  hand-raised  at  another 
institution  and  therefore  imprinted  strongly  on  her  human  caregivers.  Walnut  directed  all  her  social 
behavior  towards  her  keepers  and  never  learned  to  recognize  or  treat  other  cranes  as  her  fellow 
species.  Walnut  was  not  only  intolerant  of  other  cranes;  she  was  downright  hostile  and  killed  several 
males  during  attempts  to  pair  her  with  male  cranes  at  other  zoos.  Because  of  her  imprinted  nature, 
Walnut  had  never  produced  any  offspring  when  she  arrived  here  at  the  age  of  23.  At  the  time,  this 
made  her  the  most  genetically  valuable  White-naped  crane  in  captivity.  We  knew  right  away  she 
would  have  to  be  housed  alone  and  never  paired  with  a male.  Through  artificial  insemination  we 
produced  four  female  chicks  from  her,  two  in  2005,  one  in  2007  and  one  in  2008.  In  2007  we  thought 
of  a way  to  improve  both  the  efficiency  of  A.I.  and  Walnut’s  quality  of  life  during  the  A.I. 

The  imprinted  Walnut  would  always  greet  her  keepers  very  excitedly,  especially  during  the  breeding 
season.  I am  her  primary  keeper  and  eventually  Walnut  began  to  take  a special  interest  in  me  and  I 
became  the  exclusive  target  of  her  courtship  displays.  She  would  dance  around  me;  head  bob,  toss 
objects  and  elicit  her  half  of  a unison  call.  Despite  my  best  efforts  I was  never  able  to  replicate  my 
half  of  the  unison  call,  but  I could  at  least  bob  my  head,  give  her  mealworms  and  mice,  help  her  build 
a nest,  and  praise  her  with  compliments.  Our  interactions  outside  the  breeding  season  were  much  the 
same  and  I made  a point  to  spend  time  with  her  to  increase  our  bond  and  her  comfort  level  with  me. 
Despite  her  tameness.  Walnut  would  only  allow  me  so  close  before  moving  away.  Even  at  the  height 
of  breeding  season  I could  only  approach  to  within  a few  feet  of  her  before  she  would  walk  away  and 
then  resume  her  courtship  behavior. 

I began  training  Walnut  for  A.I.  without  physical 
restraint  prior  to  the  2007  breeding  season.  I started 
by  reaching  my  hand  towards  her  while  saying  the 
word  “touch”.  Initially  she  would  step  away  before  I 
could  even  touch  her.  After  some  time  Walnut  would 
allow  my  hand  to  touch  her  back  before  walking 
away.  I would  reward  her  with  a pinkie  mouse  and 
verbal  praise.  Gradually  Walnut  learned  to  associate 
the  sound  of  the  word  “touch”  with  my  reaching  out 
to  her,  the  fact  that  I did  her  no  harm  in  touching  her, 
and  with  the  mouse  reward.  I was  eventually  able 
to  pet  her  entire  back  before  she  moved  off.  This 
all  laid  the  groundwork  for  further  contact  once 
breeding  season  began.  Walnut  would  solicit  me  to 
mate  with  her  by  turning  her  back,  lowering  her  head, 
and  opening  and  trembling  her  wings.  In  the  past, 
she  would  only  solicit  me  briefly  and  would  stop  as 
soon  as  I got  within  a few  feet.  But  after  training 
her  to  allow  me  to  touch  her  back  with  my  hand,  she 
became  much  more  accommodating  and  comfortable. 
Walnut  began  to  allow  me  to  touch  her  legs  and  later 
manually  massage  them  as  is  necessary  to  stimulate 
her  for  A.I.  It  was  a gradual  process  of  her  allowing 
longer  and  more  contact  each  day.  Once  again,  her 
rewards  for  the  desired  behavior  of  standing  still  were  pinkie  mice  and  verbal  praise. 

At  the  peak  of  the  breeding  season,  the  actual  physical  contact  became  a reward  in  itself  for  Walnut 
who  would  purr  loudly  and  the  pinkie  mice  rewards  were  no  longer  necessary.  The  next  step  was  to 
massage  toward  her  cloaca  with  one  hand  while  holding  a syringe  in  the  other.  This  required  that  I 
stop  massaging  her  thighs  which  was  at  first  very  disappointing  for  Walnut  and  would  cause  her  to 


Animal  Keepers'  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5  179 


walk  away  as  soon  as  I stopped.  I learned  to  transition  from  massaging  her  thigh  with  both  hands 
to  massaging  her  cloaca  with  one  hand  quickly  enough  that  she  stayed  receptive  and  stationary. 
This  physical  stimulation  causes  a female  crane’s  cloaca  to  pulsate,  indicating  her  readiness  to 
accept  breeding  which  in  this  case  was  an  empty  syringe  that  would  be  complete  with  a semen 
sample  in  the  future  once  Walnut  was  trained.  Once  stimulated  enough,  Walnut  would  accept  the 
syringe  and  insemination  could  be  conducted.  I would  gently  spin  the  syringe  into  the  left  side  of 
her  now  naturally  lubricated  cloaca.  While  continuing  the  stimulation,  I would  inject  the  syringe. 
The  stimulation  continued  until  Walnut  would  step  away  and  the  syringe  fell  out  on  its  own.  Once 
Walnut  accepted  this  entire  process  we  began  to  collect  semen  samples  from  her  SSP®  declared  mate 
and  I conducted  the  actual  A.I. 

The  success  of  her  training  came  to  fruition 
when  Walnut  laid  fertile  eggs.  Her  eggs 
were  given  to  an  experienced  pair  of  White- 
naped  cranes  to  incubate,  hatch  and  raise  as 
their  own.  These  experienced  birds  were 
Abigail  and  Ray,  who  was  the  biological 
father  of  the  two  chicks.  We  did  not  leave 
the  egg  with  Walnut  due  to  her  imprinted 
nature  and  to  spare  her  the  difficulties  of 
having  to  do  the  incubating  and  rearing  job 
of  two  cranes.  Walnut  defended  her  eggs 
when  we  came  to  take  them  from  her  but 
would  recover  her  friendly  nature  soon 
afterwards.  The  success  of  her  training  had 
the  unintended  results  of  causing  Walnut  to 
continue  to  solicit  even  moments  after  an  insemination.  She  clearly  enjoyed  the  process  and  would 
solicit  me  multiple  times  per  day.  It  also  increased  her  normal  crane  territoriality  to  a degree  that 
caused  her  to  attack  any  keeper  other  than  me.  Needless  to  say,  the  entire  process  and  results  made 
Walnut  and  I a frequent  topic  of  amusement  amongst  my  co-workers. 

Walnut  was  sent  to  NZP-CRC  because  other  zoos  had  failed  to  breed  her  and  through  our  efforts 
she  has  produced  four  valuable  female  chicks.  Her  genetic  representation  in  the  population  is  now 
sufficient  enough  to  preclude  the  need  for  additional  offspring  in  the  near  future.  But  Walnut  and  I 
continue  to  enjoy  each  other’s  company  throughout  the  year  and  go  through  A.I.  training  every  time 
she  solicits  me  during  the  breeding  season.  In  2009,  Walnut  became  a grandmother  when  her  2007 
chick  had  a chick  of  her  own  while  paired  with  a male  at  the  Toledo  Zoo. 

^^Amanda^^  the  White-naped  Crane 

Amanda  the  White-naped  crane  was  19  years  old  when  she  arrived  at  NZP-CRC  in  2008.  She  had 
lost  her  mate  at  another  zoo  and  was  sent  here  for  re-pairing,  Amanda  had  been  housed  together  with 
her  prior  mate  and  laid  eggs,  but  had  never  hatched  any  chicks.  Soon  after  her  arrival,  we  began  to 
understand  why.  Amanda  would  approach  us  closer  than  our  other  cranes  (except  for  Walnut)  and 
exhibited  a tameness  that  revealed  she  must  have  been  hand-raised  and  imprinted  on  people.  Once 
breeding  season  rolled  around,  Amanda  displayed  courtship  behavior  towards  her  keepers  by  dancing 
and  head-bobbing.  She  would  also  briefly  solicit  both  male  and  female  keepers  removing  any  doubt 
that  she  was  imprinted. 

Because  of  her  previous  pairing  we  were  willing  to  try  carefully  pairing  Amanda  here,  but  she  had 
arrived  too  late  in  2008  to  be  paired.  As  a back-up  plan  for  the  next  breeding  season  I began  training 
her  to  accept  A.I.  without  any  restraint.  Amanda  was  much  more  tolerant  to  my  approach  and 
physical  contact  than  Walnut.  A process  that  took  months  with  Walnut  was  accomplished  in  weeks 
with  Amanda.  Her  rapid  acceptance  of  the  stimulation  and  insemination  process  was  hampered  by 


180  Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5 


one  quality  that  even  the  notoriously  ill-tempered  Walnut,  who  remember  had  killed  two  male  cranes, 
did  not  possess.  Amanda  would  occasionally  and  without  warning  go  from  purring  with  enjoyment 
at  being  stimulated  to  turning  around  and  pecking  at  my  face.  During  crane  A.I.  it  is  necessary  for 
me  to  get  down  on  one  or  both  knees  to  be  at  the  proper  height  to  see  what  I’m  doing.  This  puts  me 
at  a very  vulnerable  position  should  the  soliciting  female  have  a sudden  and  violent  change  of  mind. 
Once  Amanda  turned  around  she  would  not  only  be  inches  away  but  also  be  striking  at  me  from 
above.  ThankMly  over  time,  and  after  I donned  protective  eyewear,  Amanda’s  attacks  decreased 
and  eventually  disappeared  entirely. 

My  guess  is  that  despite  her  seemingly  quick  acceptance  of  the  process,  Amanda  was  not  as  comfortable 
as  she  appeared  and  her  attacks  were  signaling  such  discomfort.  Where  Walnut  would  step  away 
when  uncomfortable,  perhaps  Amanda  would  attack.  I continued  to  train  Amanda  whenever  she 
would  solicit  me  and  also  provided  her  with  nesting  material  that  often  seemed  to  be  her  most  prized 
reward.  There  was  one  very  awkward  moment  when  I mistakenly  called  her  “Walnut”.  Whether 
Amanda  understood  my  distraction  or  not,  she  clearly  recognized  it  as  a word  or  sound  she  hadn’t 
heard  from  me  before.  Amanda  immediately  stopped  soliciting  and  abruptly  turned  back  to  look  at 
me.  If  there  was  ever  a time  for  her  to  attack  me  that  was  it,  but  I was  thankfully  able  to  woo  her 
back  quickly  with  some  mice. 

Unlike  Walnut,  we  were  willing  to  attempt  to  pair  Amanda  with  a male  recommended  by  the  SSP® 
in  2009.  Amanda  and  the  male  were  initially  housed  side  by  side  in  adjacent  yards  to  monitor 
their  interactions  along  the  common  fence  line.  They  began  unison  calling  prior  to  breeding  season 
and  frequently  danced  and  head-bobbed.  Once  she  was  alongside  the  male  crane,  Amanda  stopped 
displaying  courtship  behavior  towards  me  but  retained  her  tameness  and  tolerance  of  my  close 
approach.  Amanda’s  imprinted  nature  created  the  possibility  the  crane  pair  would  not  get  along,  but 
we  observed  no  aggression  or  displacement  behavior  between  the  two  and  allowed  them  supervised 
access  to  the  same  pen.  Once  breeding  season  began,  Amanda  became  less  tolerant  of  my  approach 
and  would  frequently  make  threat  displays  towards  me.  We  still  kept  a close  eye  on  the  pair  and 
monitored  them  for  any  incompatibility. 

One  afternoon  I checked  on  the  pair  and  found  Amanda  hiding  in  the  ornamental  grasses  in  a comer 
of  her  pen.  This  is  never  a good  sign  and  I quickly  herded  the  indifferent  male  into  a separate  pen. 
As  I approached  Amanda,  she  slowly  stood  up  and  her  body  was  quivering.  There  were  no  wounds 
visible  but  she  was  clearly  traumatized.  After  ignoring  my  presence  while  getting  along  with  the 
male,  she  now  stepped  right  up  to  me  seeking  reassurance.  She  allowed  me  to  place  my  hand  on  her 
back  as  I examined  her.  After  an  exam  by  our  veterinarian  it  was  determined  that  Amanda  had  no 
physical  injuries  but  her  behavior  clearly  indicated  she  had  been  on  the  receiving  end  of  the  male’s 
aggression.  We  suspected  that  her  imprinted  nature  might  have  played  a role  in  the  pair’s  sudden 
incompatibility.  Amanda  and/or  the  male  crane  may  have  gotten  their  signals  crossed  or  the  male 
may  just  have  been  too  eager  to  mate  while  she  wasn’t  fully  receptive  yet.  Amanda  remained  stressed 
for  several  days  and  I soothed  her  by  keeping  her  company  and  offering  her  favorite  food,  dead 
pinkie  mice.  The  male  was  moved  out  of  view  from  Amanda  to  facilitate  her  recovery. 

After  a few  days,  Amanda  began  greeting  my  approach  with  dancing  and  head-bobbing  and 
eventually  soliciting  for  me  to  mate  with  her.  At  the  end  of  the  previous  breeding  season  and  prior 
to  her  introduction  to  the  male  crane,  I had  trained  her  to  accept  A.I.  without  any  physical  restraint. 
Remarkably,  we  were  able  to  pick  right  up  where  we  had  left  off  We  began  semen  collection  from 
her  abusive  mate  and  inseminating  Amanda  in  hopes  of  producing  her  first  ever  chick.  The  male  was 
now  housed  next  to  a younger  female  both  for  companionship  and  to  keep  his  hormones  at  breeding 
levels.  A few  weeks  after  we  began  collecting  semen,  the  younger  female  next  door  was  sent  to 
another  zoo  to  be  paired  with  another  male.  Now  housed  away  from  both  Amanda  and  his  younger 
companion,  the  male  stopped  producing  good  semen  samples. 


Animal  Keepers'  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5  181 


As  an  insurance  policy  against  an  unsuccessful  insemination  or  an  embryo  failing  to  survive  to 
hatching,  we  keep  attempting  A.I.  whenever  possible  until  an  egg  hatches  successfully.  We  needed 
to  move  the  male  next  door  to  a single  female  in  hopes  of  bringing  him  back  into  breeding  mode. 
Unfortunately  the  only  single  female  cranes  we  had  were  his  ex,  Amanda,  and  Walnut,  who  had  killed 
males  at  another  zoo.  Unwilling  to  tempt  Walnut’s  vengeance,  we  reluctantly  moved  the  male  back 
next  door  to  Amanda  and  monitored  them  closely.  We  would  only  leave  him  next  door  if  Amanda 
approved  and  despite  their  past  trouble  she  was  not  at  all  troubled  by  his  presence.  Amanda  did  not 
begin  returning  the  male’s  instant  courtship  displays  but  would  comfortably  hang  out  with  the  male 
along  the  common  fence  line  of  their  adjacent  pens. 

Though  we  conducted  many  inseminations  to  improve  our  chances  for  producing  a chick,  with  great 
luck  I was  able  to  successfully  inseminate  Amanda  on  the  first  attempt.  We  took  her  egg  and  gave 
it  to  Abigail  and  Ray,  the  same  experienced  foster  pair  of  White-napped  cranes  that  had  previously 
raised  Walnut’s  chicks.  These  foster  parents  were  actually  the  parents  of  Amanda’s  estranged  mate, 
making  them  grandparents  of  the  female  chick  they  would  successfully  incubate,  hatch  and  raise. 
The  female  chick  replaced  her  biological  mother  as  the  most  genetically  valuable  White-napped 
crane  in  captivity.  Amanda  and  her  mate  are  currently  housed  side-by-side  and  getting  along  well. 
They  frequently  unison  call  and  Amanda  has  once  again  lost  interest  in  me,  except  for  viewing  me  as 
a convenient  source  of  delicious  mice.  We  expect  to  have  to  keep  them  separated  and  perform  A.I.  if 
they  are  recommended  again  as  a breeding  pair. 

Conclusion 

Breeding  any  endangered  species  is  fraught  with  difficulties  and  the  complexities  of  crane  behavior 
and  breeding  only  complicate  the  endeavor.  The  stories  of  Abigail,  Walnut,  and  Amanda  are  prime 
examples  of  the  significant  obstacles  to  improving  the  genetics  of  the  captive  population  of  White- 
naped  cranes.  Performing  A.I.,  and  A.I.  without  physical  restraint  helped  us  confront  and  overcome 
these  obstacles.  Through  A.I.  we  produced  two  chicks  from  Abigail  and  two  chicks  from  Walnut.  By 
using  A.I.  without  restraint  we  produced  two  more  chicks  from  Walnut  and  one  chick  from  Amanda. 
Our  methods  have  enabled  us  to  enhance  the  efficiency  of  A.I. , improve  our  birds’  quality  of  life, 
and  strengthen  the  genetic  stability  of  the  White-naped  crane  SSP®  population.  We  look  forward 
to  continuing  our  efforts  with  White-naped  cranes,  incorporating  those  techniques  to  other  crane 
species,  and  sharing  our  knowledge  with  colleagues. 


Keeper’s  Hand  and  Crane’s  Footprint  in  the  Snow 


All  photos  by  Chris  Crowe 


182  Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5 


Enrichment  and  Training... It’s  For  the  Birds 

By  Dawn  Neptune,  Utah’s  Hogle  Zoo;  Diane  Olsen,  Moody  Gardens;  Tammy  Root, 

Indianapolis  Zoo;  Robin  Shewokis,  The  Leather  Elves;  Catherine  Vine,  Philadelphia  Zoo 
and  Deana  Walz,  The  Living  Planet  Aquarium 

It  has  been  said  that  birds  are  one  of  the  most  challenging  groups  of  animals  to  enrich.  Many 
zoological  facilities  house  birds  in  naturalistic  exhibits  with  many  other  species  which  can  make 
it  more  complicated  to  design  a behavioral  enrichment  program  for  individual  birds.  It  may  be 
difficult  to  enrich  birds,  but  with  some  creativity,  behavioral  goal  setting,  and  an  open  mind,  it  can 
be  accomplished. 

When  you  begin  developing  an  enrichment  behavioral  husbandry  program,  you  must  start  by 
researching  the  natural  behaviors  of  your  species.  Often  it  is  this  process  that  leads  to  the  creation 
of  new  enrichment  ideas.  Once  the  natural  history  has  been  researched,  also  consider  the  individual 
history  of  the  individuals  (if  known)  as  well  as  facility  constraints.  It  is  from  here  that  you  can  begin 
to  set  goals  that  you  want  your  program  to  accomplish.  Behavioral  goal  setting  is  the  backbone  to  a 
strong,  well-rounded  behavioral  husbandry  program. 

Training  is  also  an  integral  part  of  a balanced  behavioral  husbandry  program.  It  becomes  very  useful 
when  establishing  a core  set  of  husbandry  behaviors.  During  stressfiil  events  such  as  moves  to  new 
exhibits  or  veterinary  exams,  enriched  birds  tend  to  be  calmer  and  recover  more  quickly.  Training 
birds  for  husbandry  behaviors  such  as  crating  puts  a lot  less  stress  on  the  keeper  and  the  bird  during 
these  situations.  In  addition,  training  and  enrichment  can  facilitate  breeding  by  increasing  comfort 
in  the  environment,  decreasing  aggression  between  potential  pairs,  and  in  some  cases  even  lead  to 
successffil  artificial  insemination.  As  wild  bird  populations  continue  to  decrease  at  an  alarming  rate, 
keeping  and  breeding  healthy  birds  that  retain  a large  repertoire  of  natural  behaviors  may  very  well 
be  essential  to  the  preservation  of  many  avian  species. 

To  get  the  brainstorming  process  started,  we  have  given  you  some  suggestions  for  taxon  behavioral 
goals,  as  well  as  some  enrichment  and  training  ideas  to  help  you  achieve  them.  Remember,  it  is 
extremely  important  to  observe  any  enrichment  activity  for  any  problems  and  to  ensure  you  are 
targeting  the  goals  set  in  your  program.  The  safety  of  all  enrichment  should  be  carefully  considered, 
and  birds  should  be  monitored  with  any  new  enrichment. 

Passerines 

Behavioral  goals:  nest  building,  perching,  vocalizing,  foraging,  courtship,  preening,  bathing, 
husbandry  training 

Enrichment  ideas  to  achieve  behavioral  goals: 

❖ Nest  Building: 

• Provide  a variety  of  nesting  material  (grasses,  raffia,  wood  wool,  branches,  nest  cups, 
platforms,  hay), 

• Adjust  perching  to  facilitate  nest  structures  as  needed 

• Create  visual  barriers  from  the  public  and  staff  to  accommodate  shyer,  more  secretive 
birds. 

❖ Perching: 

• Provide  a variety  of  perching  types  (grapevine,  horizontal  perches,  branching  perches) 
at  a variety  of  heights 

• Rotate  perching 

• Provide  trees  and  other  plants  for  perching 
Vocalizing: 

• Play  recorded  sounds  of  same  and  different  species 

• Hang  bird-safe  mirrors 

• Provide  visual  access  to  birds  of  same  and  other  species  in  separate  enclosures 


Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5  183 


A green  aracari  checks  out  a fruit 

kabob.  (Photo:  Catherine  Vine) 


A penguin  finishes  his  “painting”  enrichment  - 
note  paint  footprints  on  floor.  {Photo:  Jenny  Thompson) 


One  option  for  a bath  pan  for  birds  in 

holding.  (Photo:  Catherine  Vine) 


Plastic  storage  containers  make  great  pools  for 
small  ducks  in  holding.  (Photo:  Catherine  Vine) 


Parrot  toys  and  whole  fruit 
are  great  enrichment  for 
toucanets  and  aracaris. 

(Photo:  Catherine  Vine) 


184  Animal  Keepers'  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5 


❖ Foraging: 

• Provide  live  insects  for  insectivorous  birds 

• Cut  fruit  in  large  chunks  and  spike  on  kabobs  or  on  perches 

• Sprinkle  seed  on  the  cut  end  of  a head  of  lettuce  and  spike  on  perches  (for  seed  eaters) 

• Hide  mealworms  or  waxworms  in  the  cut  end  of  a head  of  lettuce  and  spike  on  perches 
(for  insectivorous  birds) 

• Use  PVC  or  plastic  puzzle  feeders  with  holes  to  hide  insects  or  seed  (Wiffle®  balls  also 
work;  PVC  may  be  burned  for  a “natural”  appearance  if  needed) 

• Sprinkle  insects  in  water  features  (for  insectivorous  birds) 

• Provide  different  types  of  fruit  and  rotate  on  different  days  (there  are  many  frozen 
fruits  readily  available  that  work  well  for  passerines,  such  as  peas,  com,  blueberries, 
blackberries,  etc.) 

• Change  the  location  of  feeders 

• Toss  insects  in  air  for  aerial  hunters 

❖ Courtship: 

• Provide  conspecifics  if  possible 

• Play  recordings  of  same  species 

• Provide  a bird-safe  mirror  for  single  birds 

• Provide  a “flush”  of  insects  (many  birds  increase  protein  during  courtship  and  while 
raising  chicks) 

❖ Preening: 

• Provide  water  features  or  dishes  that  allow  for  bathing 

• Provide  a mister/sprinkler  for  bathing 

• Provide  preening  toys  (cotton  or  sisal  rope) 

• Provide  sand  or  dirt  for  bathing 

❖ Bathing: 

• Provide  water  features  or  dishes  that  allow  for  bathing 

• Provide  water  mister/sprinklers  (automated  or  motion  sensor) 

❖ Training: 

• Hand-feed 

• Scale 

• Crate/shift/trap  cage 

• Target  to  object  or  finger  point/station 

Psittacines 

Behavioral  goals:  social,  challenge/task  oriented,  preening/bathing,  nest  building/chewing, 
vocalizing,  foraging,  husbandry  training 

Enrichment  ideas  to  achieve  behavioral  goals: 

❖ Social 

• Spend  time  with  socialized  birds  throughout  the  day  (training  sessions,  etc.) 

• Provide  visual  access  to  other  psittacines  (if  possible) 

• House  in  pairs  or  family  groups  (if  possible) 

❖ Challenge/task  oriented 

• Provide  with  puzzle  feeders,  boxes,  mirrors,  toys  with  a variety  of  materials,  etc. 

• Shape  the  skills  necessary  for  the  bird  to  complete  the  task  for  complex  feeders  (if 
needed) 

❖ Preening/Bathing 

• Provide  water  features  or  dishes  that  allow  for  bathing 

• Provide  water  mister/sprinklers  (automated  or  motion  sensor) 

❖ Nest  Building/Chewing 

• Provide  with  a variety  of  nesting  materials 

• Provide  visual  barriers  (plants,  screens,  etc.)  for  shyer  birds 

• Provide  a variety  of  chewable  toys  or  chunks  of  wood 

• Provide  with  nest  logs  and  cavities 

❖ Vocalizing 

• Provide  visual  access  to  other  birds 

• Play  recorded  sounds  of  a variety  of  bird  species 

❖ Foraging 

• Scatter  food  within  the  enclosure,  both  on  ground  and  spiked  or  hung  on  perching 

• Rotate  food  options 

• Provide  with  whole  nuts 


Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  VoL  37  No.  4/5  185 


❖ Training 

• Hand  feed 

• Scale 

• Crate 

• Shift 

• Sit  on  hand  (if  desired  for  demonstrations,  etc.) 

• Fly  to  hand  (if  desired  for  demonstrations,  etc.) 

• Blood  draws,  cloacal/choanal  swabs,  and  other  veterinary  procedures 

Raptors 

Behavioral  goals:  roosting,  hunting,  manipulation,  husbandry  training 

Enrichment  ideas  to  achieve  behavioral  goals: 

❖ Roosting 

• Provide  variety  of  perches  for  bird  to  choose  from 

• Provide  perching  behind  sheltered  areas  for  privacy 

• Screen  roosting  area  with  live  or  artificial  plants 

❖ Hunting 

• Play  audio  recordings  of  prey  species 

• Change  location  of  provided  food 

• Hide  food  under  natural  (branches,  stumps,  etc.)  or  artificial  (boxes,  phone  books,  etc.) 
objects 

❖ Manipulation 

• Provide  varied  perching  with  different  wrapping  on  the  perch  (sisal  rope,  Astroturf®) 

• Provide  various  foot  toys  (Kong®  toys,  etc) 

❖ Training: 

• Scale 

• Crate 

• Voluntary  beak  and  nail  trims 

• Voluntarily  allow  jesses  and  leashes  (if  desired  for  programs,  etc.) 

• Step  onto  hand  (if  desired  for  programs,  etc.) 

• Fly  to  and  from  hand  and  perches  (if  desired  for  programs,  etc.) 

Penguins 

Behavioral  goals:  swimming,  exploration,  nesting/breeding,  husbandry  training 

Enrichment  ideas  to  achieve  behavioral  goals: 

❖ Swimming 

• Free  feed  in  water  to  entice  bird  to  swim 

• Train  bird  to  come  to  a whistle  for  food  in  the  water 

❖ Exploration 

• Place  items  of  interest,  such  as  mirrors,  Wififle®  balls,  mobiles  and  chalk  drawings  in 
various  parts  of  the  exhibit  or  holding. 

• Place  frozen  ice  blocks  and  artificial  kelp  in  various  parts  of  the  exhibit 

• Provide  kiddie  pools  with  ice  water 

• Provide  towels  (no  frayed  edges  or  holes) 

❖ Breeding 

• Provide  nesting  materials  (grass,  leaves)/boxes/substrates  (dirt,  sand) 

• Provide  a variety  of  different  nest  rock  or  box  set-ups 

❖ Training 

• Shifting 

• Scale 

• Feet  present 

• Crate 

• Voluntary  blood/semen  collection/artificial  insemination 

Toucans,  Aragarig,  Toucanets 

Behavioral  goals:  foraging,  play/manipulation,  breeding,  social  opportunities,  training. 

Enrichment  ideas  to  achieve  behavioral  goals: 

<♦  Foraging 

• Place  favorite  treats  in  various  parts  of  the  exhibit 

• Cut  fruit  in  large  chunks  and  spike  on  kabobs  or  branches 

• Rotate  feeders 


186  Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  VoL  37  No.  4/5 


• Scatter/hide  insects,  toss  insects  in  air 

• Provide  insect  puzzle  feeders 

❖ Play/Manipulation: 

• Provide  hanging  parrot  toys 

• Off-exhibit:  acrylic,  bells,  sisal  rope,  beads,  wooden  blocks,  plastic  rings,  Dixie®  cups, 
Easter  eggs,  balls 

• On-exhibit:  natural  wooden  blocks,  sisal  rope,  leather,  bamboo,  pieces  of  perching 

• Provide  with  live  plants 

• Provide  mobiles 

❖ Breeding 

• Provide  a variety  of  nest  logs  and  cavities 

• Provide  nesting  material 

❖ Social  opportunities 

• Provide  with  opportunities  to  see  other  birds  (if  possible) 

• Provide  with  bird-safe  mirrors 

❖ Training 

• Hand-feed 

• Scale 

• Crate/shift 

• Target/Station 

• Trade  (if  birds  gain  access  to  dangerous  objects) 

Waterfowl 

Behavioral  goals:  swimming/bathing,  preening,  foraging,  social,  breeding,  training 

Enrichment  ideas  to  achieve  behavioral  goals: 

❖ Swimming/Bathing: 

• Provide  with  large  water  tub  if  off-exhibit 

• Provide  with  a large  water  feature  at  least  8 inches  deep  (~20cm)  if  on-exhibit 

❖ Preening: 

• Provide  with  bathing  opportunities 

• Provide  a mister  or  offer  bath  from  hose  (mist  setting) 

❖ Foraging: 

• Scatter  food  on  ground 

• Scatter  food  in  water 

• Provide  with  whole  lettuce  heads 

• Scatter  insects  on  land  or  in  water 

• Provide  mud  area  for  dabbling 

• Toss  fish  in  water  for  mergansers  and  other  appropriate  species 

• Sink  and  weigh  down  food  under  water  for  diving  ducks 

❖ Social: 

• Keep  in  groups  of  same  species  if  possible 

• Keep  with  other  waterfowl  if  possible 

• Provide  bird-safe  mirror  for  single  birds 

❖ Breeding 

• Provide  various  substrates  and/or  nest  boxes 

• Provide  planted  areas  for  birds  to  hide 

❖ Training 

• Scale 

• Crate/Shift 

• Target/Station 
Cglumbiformes 

Behavioral  goals:  nest  building,  perching,  vocalizing,  foraging,  courtship,  preening/bathing, 
training. 

Enrichment  ideas  to  achieve  behavioral  goals: 

❖ Nest  building: 

• Provide  nest  baskets  or  nest  platforms 

• Provide  nesting  material  (grass,  sticks,  hay,  plant  trimmings) 

❖ Perching: 

• Provide  a variety  of  perching  types  (grapevine,  horizontal  perches,  branching  perches) 


Animal  Keepers'  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5  187 


• Rotate  perching 

• Provide  trees  and  other  plants  for  perching 

❖ Vocalizing: 

• Play  recorded  sounds  of  same  and  different  species 

• Hang  bird- safe  mirrors 

• Provide  visual  access  to  birds  of  same  and  other  species  in  separate  enclosures 

❖ Foraging: 

• Sprinkle  seed  on  cut  end  of  lettuce  heads  and  spike  on  branches  (for  seed  eaters) 

• Cut  fruit  in  large  chunks  and  spike  on  branches  (for  fruit  eaters) 

• Scatter  seed  on  ground  (for  seed  eaters) 

• Change  locations  of  feeders 

❖ Courtship: 

• Provide  conspecifics  if  possible 

• Play  recordings  of  same  species 

• Provide  a bird-safe  mirror  for  single  birds 

❖ Preening/Bathing: 

• Provide  water  features  or  dishes  that  allow  for  bathing 

• Provide  a mister/sprinkler  for  bathing 

❖ Training: 

• Scale 

• Crate/Shift 

• Station 
Coraciformes 

Behavioral  goals:  nest  building,  perching,  vocalizing,  foraging,  courtship,  preening/bathing, 
training 

Enrichment  ideas  to  achieve  behavioral  goals: 

❖ Nest  building: 

• Provide  nest  cavities  or  soft  logs 

• Provide  nesting  material  (grass,  sticks,  hay,  plant  trimmings) 

❖ Perching: 

• Provide  a variety  of  perching  types  (grapevine,  horizontal  perches,  branching  perches) 

• Rotate  perching 

• Provide  trees  and  other  plants  for  perching 

❖ Vocalizing: 

• Play  recorded  sounds  of  same  and  different  species 

• Hang  bird-safe  mirrors 

• Provide  visual  access  to  birds  of  same  and  other  species  in  separate  enclosures 

❖ Foraging: 

• Toss  insects  in  air  for  aerial  hunters 

• Toss  fish  in  water  (for  kingfishers) 

• Provide  live  insects 

• Change  locations  of  feeders 

❖ Courtship: 

• Provide  conspecifics  if  possible 

• Play  recordings  of  same  species 

• Provide  a bird-safe  mirror  for  single  birds 

❖ Preening/Bathing: 

• Provide  water  features  or  dishes  that  allow  for  bathing 

• Provide  a mister/sprinkler  for  bathing 

❖ Training: 

• Scale 

• Crate/Shift 

Training  can  be  an  integral  part  of  a balanced  Environmental  Enrichment  program.  It  becomes  very  useful 
when  establishing  a core  set  of  husbandry  behaviors.  During  that  dreadful  veterinary  exam,  your  bird  can 
be  calm  and  collected  if  he  or  she  understands  what  a scale  or  even  a stethoscope  is.  Training  birds  for  these 
particular  behaviors  puts  a lot  less  stress  on  the  keeper  and  the  bird  during  their  exams. 


188  Animal  Keepers'  Forum,  VoL  37  No.  4/5 


If  you  are  new  to  bird  training  or  just  need  a refresher,  below  are  some  ideas  for  bird  training 
behaviors.  Please  be  aware  that  these  are  just  the  very  basics  and  within  the  steps  of  each  behavior 
and  depending  on  your  bird,  you  may  have  to  take  smaller  approximations. 

Scale  Training 

• Show  bird  scale 

• Put  treat  on  scale 

• Give  treat  to  bird  as  bird  stands  on  scale 
Blood  draws 

• Drawing  blood  from  sinus  just  above  tail 

• Applying  pressure  on  sinus  with  finger 

• Applying  pressure  on  sinus  with  end  of  paper  clip  to  act  as  a “needle” 

• Drawing  blood  from  jugular 

• Apply  pressure  on  neck 

• Gradually  get  bird  comfortable  to  neck  being  stretched 

• Apply  pressure  on  jugular  with  end  of  paper  clip  to  act  as  a “needle” 

Nail  Trims 

• Get  bird  comfortable  on  platform  that  nails  will  be  trimmed  on 

• Get  bird  comfortable  with  feet  being  held 

• Start  slowly  and  trim  just  1-2  nails  at  a time 

• Gradually  get  bird  to  station  long  enough  to  trim  all  nails 
Wing  Clips 

• Station  bird 

• Get  bird  used  to  wings  being  lifted  by  trainers 

• Get  bird  used  to  seeing  scissors 

• Gradually  get  bird  used  to  feathers  being  trimmed 

Tactile 

• The  more  you  can  handle  your  bird,  the  better!  This  allows  keepers  to  be  able  to  check 
preening  glands,  molting  issues,  feet  problems,  etc,  etc. 


Recommended  websites  for  a number  of  great  devices: 

http://www.chopperstoys.com/CatalogNaturals.html  (love  their  'Nature's  Naturals’!) 

http  ://www.  estarbird.com 

http://www.theleatherelves.com/main/index.php 

http://www.busybeaks.com 

http  ://www.birdsjustwannahavefian.com 

http  ://www.naturesinstinct.com 

http  ://www,parrottoy.  com 

http  ://www.  twinleather.com 

http  ://www.  creativeforagingsy  stems . com/products  .html 
http://www,fauna"tek.net/parrot_models.htm 

Other  good  enrichment  websites: 

http://www.animaltraining.org 

http://www.aazk.org/committees/enrichment/comm_enrichment_birds.php 


Animal  Keepers'  Forum,  VoL  37  No.  4/5  189 


Jess  Free 


By  Stephanie  Ashley,  Bird  Programs  Manager 
Tracy  Aviary,  Salt  Lake  City,  UT 


Bird  training  is  a difficult  venture.  Here  is  an  animal  that  thinks  and  moves  differently  from  us.  It 
can  be  difficult  to  pinpoint  a bird’s  motivation,  and  even  more  difficult  to  secure  it.  When  you  add 
that  to  the  challenge  of  working  with  an  animal  that  can  simply  fly  away  from  you,  it  is  easy  to 
understand  why  so  many  of  us  depend  on  physical  control. 

One  of  the  most  common  forms  of  physical  control  seen  in  modem  (and  ancient)  bird  training  is 
the  use  of  “jesses”  - those  long  leather  straps  which  can  be  attached  to  a bird’s  legs  and  held  by 
a handler.  It  isn’t  the  only  method  used,  though.  Bird  trainers  also  commonly  practice  holding  a 
bird’s  toe  or  foot  to  keep  it  from  flying  away.  It  is  also  common  to  trim  flight  feathers  and,  for  some 
species,  even  surgically  remove  part  of  the  wing.  And  we  do  all  this  for  one  specific  purpose:  to 
have  complete  control  over  a bird’s  ability  to  fly  away  from  us. 

When  I was  introduced  to  the  world  of  professional  bird  training  five  years  ago,  I had  already 
dabbled  as  an  amateur.  I had  learned  to  clip  my  own  pet  birds’  wings,  and  I was  proficient  at  using 
a toe  hold  on  a parrot  and  applying  jesses  to  a Kookaburra  {Dacelo  novaeguineae)  as  a docent  at  the 
local  zoo.  My  education  continued  as  a new  professional 
with  jessed  raptors,  a Kookaburra,  a Black-billed 
magpie  {Pica  pica),  and  a Red-crested  turaco  {Tauraco 
erythrolophus).  I also  learned  how  to  use  a foot  hold  on 
our  American  crow  {Corvus  brachyrhynchus),  and  toe 
holds  were  still  mandatory  for  parrots.  I quickly  learned 
not  to  tmst  the  birds,  because  they  might  fly  away.  The 
big  exceptions  were  the  vultures.  I have  never  worked 
a vulture  with  jesses,  and  was  taught  other  ways  of 
managing  them. 

Then,  in  2007  our  world  of  perfect  control  was  rocked 
by  training  consultant  Phung  Luu,  who  was  hired  by 
Tracy  Aviary  to  teach  us  to  be  better  trainers.  One  of  his 
initial  recommendations  was  that  we  remove  the  jesses 
from  our  magpie,  kookaburra,  and  turaco.  The  reasoning 
was  simple:  their  legs  are  stmctured  differently  from 
the  tendon-and-muscle  reinforced  legs  of  a raptor,  and 
they  could  be  injured.  We  supposed  that  we  had  always 
known  this,  or  why  would  we  have  a policy  of  cradling  these  bating  birds  in  our  free  hand?  We 
made  this  change  immediately,  but  were  nervous  about  it.  What  would  happen  when  we  removed 
our  safety  net? 

As  it  turns  out,  when  you  teach  a bird  the  right  behavior  and  pair  it  with  the  right  motivation  and 
reinforcement,  they  can  learn  to:  A)  not  fly  away  and  B)  come  back  when  they  do  fly  away.  Suddenly, 
our  birds  were  able  to  make  choices.  Real  choices.  And  somehow,  that  increased  their  confidence 
in  themselves  and  in  us.  The  results  were  remarkable:  birds  that  didn’t  bate  and  rarely  flew  away. 
When  they  did  fly  away,  they  were  quicker  to  return 

That  same  year,  Phung  was  working  on  a training  project  with  our  crow.  He  was  teaching  her  to  fish 
out  a piece  of  meat  with  a hook.  At  the  same  time,  we  were  preparing  her  routine  for  our  summer 
show.  During  one  of  our  sessions  on  stage,  the  crow  decided  to  fly  into  a tree.  We  immediately 
sprang  into  action,  trying  to  coax  her  down,  knowing  that  as  soon  as  she  flew  to  a hand  we  would 
seize  her  foot  and  she  would  be  trapped.  She  knew  this,  too,  and  stayed  a careful  distance  away  from 
us.  In  the  meantime,  Phung  climbed  onto  the  roof  with  a few  pieces  of  her  food,  and  quietly  called 
her.  To  my  amazement,  the  crow  responded  to  him. 


Our  King  Vulture  {Sarcoramphus  papa) 
is  one  of  our  most  consistent  and  reliable 
birds.  She  is  frequently  flown  outdoors 
with  no  jesses  or  other  physical  restraints. 

(Photo:  Bruce  Thompson) 


190  Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  VoL  37  No.  4/5 


As  I watched  our  crow  fly  to  Phung,  a part  of  me  was  screaming  at  him  to  grab  her  before  she  got 
away  again.  He  did  not.  The  crow  went  to  him,  took  a small  piece  of  food,  and  then  flitted  off  But 
she  didn’t  go  quite  as  far  as  she  had  gone  before,  and  when  she  returned  for  more,  she  did  it  more 
quickly  and  stayed  longer.  I will  never  forget  how  I felt  when  the  bird  I had  been  working  with 
for  nearly  three  years  chose  the  man  she  had  known  for  only  a week  over  me.  It  was  more  than 
embarrassing,  it  was  humbling.  It  took  a little  while  to  get  her  back  into  her  enclosure,  but  every  time 
she  came  down,  she  went  to  Phung  rather  than  fly  to  any  one  of  us. 

It  seems  so  obvious  in  retrospect,  but  I didn’t  realize 
until  that  day  that  jesses  and  foot  holds  are  the  same 
thing.  We  began  practicing  letting  go  indoors.  Our 
crow  was  fantastic  without  a foot  hold,  and  she 
soon  began  performing  as  well  for  us  as  she  did  for 
Phung.  We  noticed  that  during  indoor  programs  she 
would  sometimes  walk  or  fly  through  the  audience, 
exploring,  but  she  always  returned  when  we  called  her. 
Interesting. 

We  were  adventurers  in  training  who  had,  with  a great 
deal  of  assistance,  made  a new  discovery.  We  excitedly 
babbled  about  our  birds’  newfound  personalities  in  our 
short  meetings.  One  day,  after  hearing  our  excitement, 
Phung  said,  “imagine  working  raptors  without  jesses.” 

And  we  laughed  at  him.  Imagine!  After  all, 
EVERYBODY  knows  that  you’re  supposed  to  use 
jesses  with  raptors,  right?  Right?  It’s  just  the  way 
things  are.  Phung  smiled  at  us  politely  when  we  told 
him  we  weren’t  ready  for  THAT  yet.  And  we  weren’t. 

Perhaps  to  prime  us  for  being  “ready”  to  wean  ourselves  off  our  dependence  on  jesses,  he  had  us  fly 
raptors  without  touching  the  jesses.  We  learned  to  not  automatically  reach  for  the  jesses  when  the 
bird  landed  on  the  glove.  Sometimes  we  succeeded.  Sometimes  we  were  just  too  afraid  to  let  go. 

Time  passed,  and  we  didn’t  come  any  closer  to  abandoning  our  jesses.  We  acquired  a new  Turkey 
vulture  (Cathartes  aura),  and  I trained  her  in  the  way  with  which  I was  familiar:  without  jesses.  Isn’t 
it  fiinny  how  we  can  fool  ourselves  into  believing  that  one  thing  is  different  from  another?  To  another 
trainer,  a vulture  IS  a raptor,  and  requires  the  same  equipment.  How  is  working  a vulture  with  no 
jesses  different  from  working  a hawk  with  no  jesses?  I allowed  my  mind  to  trick  me  into  thinking 
they  were  different,  and  thought  nothing  more  of  it. 

We  also  acquired  an  Osprey  {Pandion  haliaetus).  We  tried,  unsuccessfully,  to  glove  train  her  with 
jesses.  It  wasn’t  until  my  co-trainer  attended  the  2009  lAATE  conference  that  we  began  to  see  things 
differently.  After  watching  an  interesting  presentation  about  a snowy  owl  that  preferred  not  to  stand 
on  the  glove,  he  wanted  to  try  applying  the  same  principle  to  our  osprey.  “After  all,  we  do  it  with 
our  vultures.” 

This  idea  has  snowballed  into  a new  goal  for  our  program:  how  can  we  become  jess  free?  Two  and 
a half  years  after  Phung  first  suggested  that  jesses  are  just  a crutch,  we  finally  understand.  We  now 
fly  our  female  Harris’  hawk  (Parabuteo  unicinctus)  entirely  without  jesses,  and  we  do  this  routinely. 
We  were  surprised  to  discover  her  in  a different  context:  what  would  she  do  if  she  could  truly  decide? 
Why,  she  would  stay  on  the  glove.  When  she  leaves  the  glove,  it  is  almost  always  to  go  to  her  next 
position,  or  to  another  trainer. 


Maleficent,  the  American  Crow,  now 
thrives  without  a foot  hold.  She  is  reliable 
both  indoors  and  out  of  doors,  and  has  been 
trained  to  both  fly-free  and  manipulate  a 
variety  of  prop  items.  (Photo:  Bmce  Thompson) 


Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5  191 


That’s  not  to  say  that  all  our  problems  are  solved.  We  have  a couple  of  owls  that  will  require  a lot  ' 
of  training  to  become  independent  of  their  jesses.  Fm  reluctant  to  release  the  toe  hold  on  a certain  j 
parrot  that  decided  to  sit  in  a tree  for  36  hours.  And  Fm  not  sure  what  to  do  about  a particular  ; 
Rough-legged  hawk  (Buteo  lagopus)  that  is  missing  an  entire  wing  and  would  risk  injury  on  the 
glove  without  the  jesses  there  to  catch  her.  But  we  are  working  on  it.  We  have  a goal  now,  and  we 
are  excited  about  it. 

Removing  jesses  may  be  an  extreme  example  of  allowing  a bird  to  make  a choice,  but  there  are  many 
ways  we  can  provide  these  opportunities.  Safety  should  always  be  given  first  priority,  and  each 
program  needs  to  adjust  its  methods  and  philosophies  based  on  its  abilities  to  continue  to  provide  a 
safe  environment  for  birds  and  guests.  Remember  that  this  philosophy  can  be  implemented  on  the 
most  basic  levels:  stepping  up  onto  the  glove,  loading  into  a crate,  and  standing  on  a scale.  My  hope 
is  that  other  programs  can  discover  what  ours  did:  birds  empowered  by  choice  are  more  confident, 
healthier,  and  happier. 


Harris’  Hawk  Cazadora  free-flies  without  jesses.  She  continues  to 
wear  alymeri  anklets  to  accommodate  jesses  for  tethering. 

(Photo:  Bruce  Thompson) 


192  Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5 


Greater  Flamingo  (Phoenicoptems  wseus)  with 
Capture  Myopathy:  Specialized  Care  & Husbandry 

By  Leanne  Blinco  and  Beth  Schille,  Department  of  Animal  Health 
Disney's  Animal  Kingdom®,  Bay  Lake,  Florida 

At  Disney’s  Animal  Kingdom®  we  have  an  extensive  preventative  medicine  program  that  is  part 
of  our  overall  animal  health  program.  Most  animals  are  scheduled  for  an  annual  wellness  exam 
which  for  birds  may  include:  a physical  exam,  body  weight  measurement,  blood  sample  collection 
and  analysis,  transponder  placement  (if  necessary),  radiographs  (bi-annually),  and  administration  of 
prophylactic  medications  or  vaccines. 

The  annual  wellness  exams  for  the  Greater  flamingos  {Phoenicoptems  roseus)  are  performed  adjacent 
to  their  exhibit  which  is  part  of  our  Kilamanjaro  Safari  guest  experience.  The  exhibit  consists  of  a 
large  pool  and  an  island  that  is  6.1  meters  in  diameter  [20  ft.].  At  the  time,  the  exhibit  housed 
21.24.4  birds.  For  their  exams,  the  birds  are  manually  captured  and  restrained  for  approximately 
15-20  minutes  each. 

The  Greater  flamingo  is  the  largest  of  all  species  of  flamingos.  They  can  be  found  in  parts  of  Africa, 
southwest  Asia,  northern  South  America,  and  southern  Europe.  Greater  flamingos  are  the  tallest  of 
all  flamingo  species,  averaging  120-145cm  in  height  [3. 9-4.7  ft.],  3.5kg  in  weight  [-7.72  lbs.],  and 
having  a wingspan  of  140-165cm  [4.6-5.4  ft.].  Their  habitat  primarily  consists  of  high  alkaline  or 
saline  shallow  lagoons,  inland  lakes,  or  mudflats.  Diet  is  varied  and  may  include  aquatic  invertebrates, 
insects,  and  plant  matter,  (del  Hoyo  & Sargatal,  1992) 

This  paper  will  discuss  the  specialized  care  and  husbandry  provided  to  a Greater  flamingo  that  was 
diagnosed  with  capture  myopathy  following  an  annual  wellness  exam. 

Capture  myopathy,  also  known  as  exertional  rhabdomyolysis,  is  a condition  in  which  there  is  muscle 
damage  as  a result  of  a period  of  strenuous  exertion  such  as  running  or  struggling.  The  time  from 
injury  to  appearance  of  clinical  signs  is  variable  from  immediate  to  several  hours.  Rarely  are  initial 
signs  of  capture  myopathy  in  birds  noted  after  24  hours.  Clinical  signs  can  range  from  peracute  death 
due  to  cardiac  failure  to  painful,  stiff  movement  of  varying  intensity.  Capture  myopathy  is  a disease 
of  muscle,  but  nerves,  bones,  tendons,  and  ligaments  can  also  be  injured  during  evasion,  capture, 
or  restraint.  If  nerves  and  tendons  of  the  distal  legs  are  involved,  it  is  not  uncommon  for  birds  to 
“knuckle  over”  intermittently  or  continuously  on  top  of  the  foot  with  subsequent  injury  to  the  skin.  If 
tendons  completely  rupture,  inability  to  stand  or  lift  a wing  may  occur.  Fracture  injuries  can  be  severe 
and  are  often  recognized  by  palpation  of  the  affected  limb/bone  and/or  radiographs. 

Diagnosis  of  capture  myopathy  can  be  difficult.  If  it  is  possible  to  touch  the  bird,  hard  and  excessively 
warm  muscles  may  be  appreciated  on  palpation.  If  the  bird  survives  with  myopathy  for  a few  days, 
reduction  of  subcutaneous  and  abdominal  fat  may  be  appreciated.  In  mammals,  discolored  urine 
due  to  excessive  myoglobin  release  is  sometimes  noted  and  is  a sign  of  impending  kidney  failure.  A 
change  in  urate  color  should  not  be  expected  in  birds  suffering  from  capture  myopathy.  Blood  work 
may  be  helpful  in  the  diagnosis  of  capture  myopathy.  Serum  chemistry  values  such  as  CPK  and  AST 
are  often  elevated  in  cases  with  extensive  muscle  damage. 

! During  a routine  capture  for  an  annual  wellness  check,  one  of  our  female  flamingos  was  transported 
to  the  hospital  for  further  diagnostics.  A second  female  was  transported  with  her  as  a “companion 
I bird”  in  the  event  that  this  bird  would  need  to  be  hospitalized  long-  term.  After  the  staff  veterinarians 
medically  discharged  the  flamingo  in  question,  both  birds  were  transported  back  to  their  exhibit. 
Upon  release  onto  the  exhibit  island,  the  companion  bird  tangled  her  legs  up  in  some  weeds  and 


Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5  193 


started  to  struggle.  Once  she  was  freed  from  the  area  she  was  unable  to  stand  on  her  own.  Following  a 
thorough  medical  examination,  including  radiographs  to  rule  out  skeletal  abnormalities,  the  bird  was 

diagnosed  with  capture  myopathy. 


The  bird  was  unable  to  support  her  own 
weight,  therefore  a sling  frame  was 
constructed  of  PVC  similar  to  that  described 
by  Ellis  and  Gee,  1996  (Figure  1).  The  sling 
offered  the  bird  support  while  allowing  her 
to  extend  her  legs.  The  dimensions  of  the 
frame  were  68.6  cm  high  x 66.0  cm  wide  x 
9 1 .4  cm  long  [2.25  ft.  x 2. 1 6 ft.  x 2.9  ft.] . The 
actual  sling  was  made  by  placing  two  towels 
together  and  layering  a foam  pad  in  between 
for  support.  There  were  three  holes  cut  into 
the  sling,  one  for  each  leg  and  one  for  the 
bird’s  tail.  The  towels  were  attached  to  the 
frame  by  using  large  spring  clamps. 


Figure  1.  Flamingo  in  Sling 


Daily  husbandry  for  the  bird  included: 
body  weight  measurement,  tube  feeding, 
medication  administration,  supportive  care 
and  physical  therapy.  The  bird  was  tube  fed 
a fish  gruel  but  was  also  offered  food  and 
water  at  all  times  while  she  was  housed  in  the 
sling.  This  was  to  help  motivate  her  to  eat  on 
her  own.  Medications  consisted  of  anti-inflammatory  drugs,  steroids,  antibiotics,  anti-fungal  drugs, 
and  fluid  therapy.  Physical  therapy  was  performed  to  keep  her  muscles  from  atrophying.  In  addition 
to  extending  her  legs  by  hand,  the  bird  was  placed  in  a large  tub  of  warm  water  2-3  times  a day  for 
hydrotherapy  sessions  (Figure  2).  Keepers  would  broadcast  food  items  into  the  water  to  encourage  a 
feeding  response.  Finally,  on  Day  16,  the  flamingo  was  able  to  stand  and  support  her  own  weight  and 
on  Day  20,  she  started  to  eat  and  drink  on  her  own. 


Once  the  bird  was  able  to  stand  for  short  periods  of  time,  the  sling  was  transformed  into  a “walker”.  | 
Wheels  were  placed  under  i' 

each  frame  leg  (Figure  3).  This 
“walker”  allowed  the  bird  to  walk 
down  the  hallway  normally  while 
the  sling  supported  her  body 
weight.  After  a couple  days  of 
using  the  walker  she  was  able  to 
walk  on  her  own  (Day  23).  Thirty- 
eight  days  after  being  admitted 
into  the  hospital,  she  was  released 
back  onto  the  flamingo  island  with 
the  rest  of  the  flock  (Table  1). 


Conclusion 

Medical  treatment  for  capture 
myopathy  is  often  difficult 
and  is  primarily  supportive  in 
nature,  allowing  opportunity  for 
the  muscles  to  heal  enough  for 
return  to  function.  The  best  way 


Figure  2.  Flamingo  during  hydrotherapy. 


194  Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5 


to  deal  with  capture  myopathy  is  to 
prevent  it  before  it  occurs.  Changes 
have  been  made  in  our  capture  and 
restraint  techniques  here  at  Disney’s 
Animal  Kingdom®  which  has 
reduced  the  amount  of  time  a bird 
is  in  hand  during  the  routine  exam 
process.  Flamingo  groups  are  now 
kept  in  makeshift  holding  pens  and 
are  isolated  individually  as  needed 
which  prevents  them  from  pacing 
and  becoming  overheated.  Due  to  the 
number  of  birds  processed  at  one  time, 
individual  stations  have  been  set  up  to 
accomplish  medical  tasks  such  as  blood 
collection,  physical  examinations,  and 
vaccinations.  The  birds  that  have  been 
processed  are  then  released  back  onto 
the  island  where  the  animal  care  team 
can  observe  them  for  any  signs  of  post 
procedure  complications. 


Figure  3.  Close-up  of  walker  for  Flamingo. 


The  sling  system  utilized  with  this  patient  has  also  been  used  for  other  species  of  birds  to  treat  injuries 
such  as  leg  fractures,  “knuckling  under”  of  the  foot  (which  requires  a plaster  “bootie”)  presumably 
due  to  nerve/tendon  damage,  and  various  other  leg  problems.  We  have  found  that  the  sling  works 
best  with  light/smaller  birds.  These  types  of  birds  are  less  likely  to  tip  the  sling  over  to  one  side  or 
the  other. 


Table  1:  Daily  Husbandry/Progress  Chart 

Event 

Date 

Elapsed  time 

Unable  to  stand 

26  March  2000 

Admitted  into  hospital 

Tube  feeding 

27  March  - 14  April 

1 9 days 

Hydrotherapy 

28  March  - 15  April 

1 9 days 

Using  “walker” 

10  April 

16  days  after  arrival  to  hospital 

Eating/Standing  in  sling 

15  April 

20  days  after  arrival  to  hospital 

Walking/Standing  w/o  sling 

17  April 

23  days  after  arrival  to  hospital 

Bird  exercised  outside 

1 8 April 

24  days  after  arrival  to  hospital 

Stand  on  own  24  hrs/day 

26  April 

30  days  after  arrival  to  hospital 

Released  from  hospital 

2 May 

38  days  after  arrival  to  hospital 

(Photos  provided  by  Leanne  Blinco  and  Beth  Schille) 

Acknowledgments 

Thank  you  to  Scott  Tidmus  and  the  West  Savannah  team  at  Disney’s  Animal  Kingdom®  for  their  help 
and  support  while  this  bird  was  in  the  veterinary  hospital.  Additional  thanks  to  Lori  Grady  and  Dr. 
Scott  Terrell  for  editing  this  paper,  and  to  Dr.  Don  Neiffer  for  providing  additional  content. 

References 

del  Hoyo  , J,,  Elliot,  A.  & Sargatal,  J.  eds.  (1992).  Flandbook  of  the  Birds  of  the  World. 

Vol.  1 . Lynx  Edicions,  Barcelona. 

Ellis,  D.,  Gee,  G.  & Mirande  C.  eds.  (1996).  Cranes:  Their  Biology,  Husbandry, 
and  Conservation.  Hancock  House  Publishers,  United  States  and 
Hancock  House  Publishers  Ltd,  Canada. 


Animal  Keepers'  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5  195 


Succsessful  Rearing  of  an  African  Sacred  Ibis 

(Threskiornis  aethiopicus)  Chick  at  the  Toronto  Zoo 


By  Lisa  Torchetti  and  Charles  Guthrie,  Keepers 
Animal  Health  Centre,  Toronto  Zoo,  Canada 
October-December  2009 

The  African  Sacred  Ibis  {Threskiornis  aethiopicus)  is  a species  of  wading  bird  of  the  ibis  family, 
Threskiomithidae.  They  are  typically  found  in  marshes,  open  moist  areas,  flooded  farmlands,  coastal 
lagoons  in  south-eastern  Iraq,  Madagascar  and  sub-Saharan  Africa. 


Housing 

The  Toronto  Zoo  currently  has  a collection  of  1.1.15  Sacred  Ibis  with  several  breeding  pairs.  The 
whole  flock  was  moved  to  temporary  holdings  in  our  Animal  Health  Centre  due  to  construction  and  i 
renovation  of  their  current  habitat  in  the  African  Rainforest  Pavilion.  It  was  during  this  move  on  6 ; 
October  2009  that  three  eggs  were  discovered  on  a nest  built  by  the  adult  breeding  pair.  One  of  the  1 
three  eggs  had  already  started  to  pip  and  was  moved  to  an  incubator  in  the  Animal  Health  Centre’s 
ICU.  The  other  two  eggs  that  had  not  started  to  pip  were  put  back  with  the  adults  in  the  new  holding  | 
with  hopes  that  they  might  resume  caring  for  them.  Due  to  the  stress  of  the  move  and  the  bird’s  new 
surroundings,  the  adults  abandoned  the  nest  and  the  eggs  were  eventually  determined  to  be  non-  i 
viable  and  were  discarded. 


ii 


Incubation 

At  1245hrs  on  6 October,  the  pipping  egg  was  set  up  in  an  incubator  with  a temperature  of  35°C 
(95°F)  and  a humidity  of  50%.  The  entire  top  of  the  egg  was  already  cracked  and  the  chick  inside  ' 
was  cheeping  and  moving  around.  The  egg  was  checked  periodically  throughout  the  day,  but  by 
1500hrs  there  was  still  no  progress.  The  egg  was  misted  lightly  with  distilled  water  and  by  ITOOhrs,  i 
the  chick  hatched  with  some  assistance.  The  chick  was  then  examined  by  the  veterinary  staff  It 
weighed  36  grams  (1.27oz.),  a slightly  lower  birth  rate  than  stated  in  literature,  (Holland,  2007), 
but  was  found  to  be  in  good  health.  The  yolk  sac  was  fully  internalized  and  the  chick  was  resting  : 
comfortably  in  a bowl  lined  with  a cotton  towel  with  a few  twigs  for  it  to  grip  and  help  keep  its  legs  i 
together  beneath  it.  The  twigs  also  prevented  the  chick  from  lying  too  flat  on  the  bottom  of  the  bowl 
and  possibly  distorting  its  beak. 


Hand  Rearing 

At  0830hrs  on  7 October  2009  the  chick  was  heard  chirping  when  staff  arrived.  It  was  moved  to  a 
brooder  with  a temperature  set  at  34°C  [93.2°F].  It  weighed  34.  Ig  [1.2oz.].  It  was  not  offered  food 
yet  to  allow  absorption  of  its  yolk  sac.  By  1145hrs  on  the  same  day,  it  was  offered  approximately 
0.5ml  of  a “slurry’  made  of  ground  up  dog  kibble  and  eye  smelt  in  a 1:2  ratio,  respectively,  with 
vitamin  E and  thiamine.  A bird  hand-rearing  formula  (CeDe  Birdfood)  was  added  to  help  thicken 
the  slurry  and  to  add  additional  vitamins  and  nutrients.  Every  time  the  chick  gaped  it  was  fed  from 
a 3ml  syringe  with  the  tip  cut  off  It  was  slow  to  eat  but  did  swallow  the  food  eventually  with  some 
guidance.  The  chick  had  difficulty  consistently  gaping  wide  enough  to  receive  the  syringe  but  the 
keepers  solved  this  issue  by  making  an  “okay”  symbol  with  their  hands  and  pointing  the  index  finger 
and  thumb  towards  their  chest.  This  created  a hole  in  which  the  chick  put  its  beak  in  to  beg.  This 
method  was  used  to  simulate  the  parents’  beak  covering  a chick’s  beak  during  feeding.  This  worked 
quite  well  and  by  the  next  feeding  it  was  already  producing  a fair  amount  of  feces/urate.  The  chick 
was  fed  every  hour  from  0700hrs  to  2200hrs  and  the  amount  increased  in  small  increments  depending 
on  the  chick’s  eagerness.  The  chick  had  gained  weight  from  34.1g  to  48.4g  by  Day  4 [1.20-1. 72oz.]. 
The  feeding  interval  was  also  increased  to  every  1 % hr  with  the  chick  being  very  eager  to  eat  and  u 
increasing  in  strength  daily.  Its  neck  muscles  were  getting  stronger  but  the  chick  was  still  unable  to 
support  its  own  head  completely.  Its  legs  were  also  getting  stronger  and  the  chick  was  starting  to 


196  Animal  Keepers'  Forum,  VoL  37  No.  4/5 


raise  itself  up  on  them  and  was  already  trying  to  climb  out  of 
its  bowl! 

By  Day  8 the  chick  was  eating  really  well  and  was  chirping 
loudly  and  impatiently  before  each  feeding.  The  amount  of 
food  given  was  10ml  and  increasing  by  around  2ml  per  day. 
The  chick’s  weight  quadrupled,  weighing  140g  [4.9oz.].  By 
this  time  the  chick’s  eyes  were  almost  completely  open  and 
it  was  showing  more  interest  in  its  surroundings,  preening  its 
towel  and  itself 


i 


On  Day  9 the  feeding  intervals  increased  to  every  two  hours 

with  the  last  feed  at  1900tirs.  The  chick  was  very  satiated  by  this  feed  and  would  sleep  the  night 

without  waking.  On  Day  12  the  chick  was  offered  food  with 
a thicker  texture  and  on  a bent  spoon  to  encourage  it  to  feed 
itself  While  it  tried  to  peck  at  the  food  on  the  spoon,  it  was 
unable  to  pick  it  up  and  drop  it  into  the  back  its  throat  to 
swallow.  It  was  still  eating  well  with  the  assistance  of  the 
syringe  and  now  weighed  303g  [10.6oz.].  By  this  time  the 
chick’s  down  was  fully  in  and  its  pinfeathers  were  growing 
so  the  brooder  was  turned  down  to  32°C  [89.6°F]. 


Chick  displaying  mouth  gap  on  Day  7 


At  Day  14  the  chick  was  strong  and  big  enough  to  be  moved 
to  a larger  brooder,  measuring  approximately  4 ft.  x 3 ft. 
X 3 ft.  [0.91m  X 1.22m  x 0.91m].  This  was  kept  at  room 
temperature  but  supplemented  with  heat  from  a heat  lamp  clamped  to  the  outside.  The  chick  was 
also  being  weaned  off  of  the  ‘slurry’  and  onto  solid  food, 
an  adult  ibis  diet  consisting  of  ground  smelt,  Toronto 
Zoo  Small  Carnivore  Diet,  chick  chow,  gelatin  powder, 
vitamins,  minerals  and  water.  This  was  offered  in  addition 
to  chopped  pieces  of  whole  eye  smelt.  The  smelt  was  also 
supplemented  with  % tablet  (25mg)  of  thiamine  and  150 
mg  of  Vitamin  E. 


African  Sacred  Ibis  chick  on  Day  14 


During  the  first  month  of  hand-rearing,  the  Toronto 
Zoo  chick’s  growth  compared  very  closely  to  that 
reported  in  the  literature  (See  Chart  1). 


All  lean  Sacred  Ibis  (.hick  on  Day  21 


I 


Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5  197 


Chart  1 


Sacred  Ibis  Growth 


O « 2 3 4 S S 7 e 3 lO  t«  12  13  i4  16  t7  to  19  20  21  22  23 


Days 

Weaning 

The  chick  stayed  in  the  brooder  for  another  nine  days  while  still  being  assist-fed.  After  that  time  ^ 
(Day  23),  it  was  moved  to  a pen  in  one  of  the  Animal  Health  Centre  holdings  in  the  company  of  a ' 
single  adult  ibis  that  was  being  held  there  for  medical  treatment  of  a traumatic  injury.  The  process 
of  weaning  it  off  hand  feeding  was  then  started.  It  still  exhibited  begging  behaviour  but  was  not  i 
competent  in  taking  food  items  and  swallowing  them  on  its  own.  We  were  unsure  how  much  of 

the  feeding  behaviour  is  ‘hard  wired’  versus  learned  in  this 
species.  Therefore  we  continued  to  assist-feed  the  chick  but 
found  it  was  not  as  content  with  this  and  began  struggling  i 
when  handled.  If  a smelt  was  placed  part  way  down  its  throat 
it  would  usually  swallow  it  but  began  more  and  more  to  either 
refuse  or  regurgitate. 

As  the  days  progressed,  the  chick  did  begin  to  feed  itself.  On 
Day  33  the  chick  ate  from  the  keepers  hand  on  its  own  twice 
and  on  Day  42,  the  chick  went  without  assist  feeding  for  the 
first  time  and  was  observed  eating  the  adult  ibis  mix  on  its 
own.  Having  a cage-mate  present  during  feeding  made  a big 
difference  as  the  chick  slowly  learned  to  feed  itself  through 
imitation. 

From  then  on  a new  Sacred  Ibis  feeding  protocol  was 
developed.  Food  items  offered  were  the  adult  ibis  mix, 
chopped  eye  smelt,  crickets  and  mealworms.  When 
available  we  would  also  offer  chopped  pinkies  and 
Toronto  Zoo  Small  Carnivore  Diet  . The  chick  now 
weighed  910g  [21bs.]  and  we  established  a target  weight 
of  1200g  [2.641bs.],  the  weight  of  its  cage-mate.  The 
young  bird  would  be  weighed  first  thing  in  the  morning 
and  at  end  of  shift.  Assist  feeding  would  only  be  done  at 
this  stage  if  weight  loss  overnight  was  greater  than  5% 

(~50  grams). 

The  chick  continued  to  gain  weight  under  this  new 


Exercising  chick’s  wings  on  Day  21 


198  Animal  Keepers’ Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5 


protocol  Our  chick's  weight  was  compared  to  the  weights  for  a similar  species;  the  Black  ibis  /Red 
napped  ibis  (Pseudibis  papillosa)  and  are  presented  in  Chart  2 below. 


Days 


It  is  interesting  to  note  in  this  chart  that  the  data  for  our  Sacred  ibis  chick  shows  a very  similar  plateau 
in  growth  approximately  between  Days  35  and  45  to  that  of  the  Black  ibis. 

The  keepers  were  happy  that  the  chick  was  eating  increasingly  more  and  more  on  its  own,  but  were  still 
concerned  by  its  slow  and  lower  growth  rate  during  this  stage  and  often  felt  it  necessary  to  continue 
to  assist-feed.  We  would  usually  assist-feed  it  fish  at  the  end  of  the  shift  to  allow  it  sustenance  for  the 
overnight  period.  However,  as  stated  in  Holland  (2007),  aviculturists  caution  overfeeding,  which  can 
result  in  too  rapid  growth,  bowed  legs  and  constipation. 

Although  up  until  Day  30,  the  Toronto  Zoo  chick  was  growing  at  a similar  rate  to  that  reported  in  the 
literature,  the  ensuing  period  of  about  one  month  was  one  of  trial.  We  felt  that  it  was  very  important 
that  the  chick  learn  to  eat  on  its  own,  sooner  rather  than  later.  According  to  the  literature  it  was 
already  beyond  the  age  at  which  it  should  have  been  weaned.  It  was  thought  that  hunger  could  be  a 
strong  motivator  but  keepers  were  concerned  that  since  we  had  failed  to  teach  it  to  take  food  on  its 
own  during  the  early  stages,  it  might  suffer  from  lack  of  nutrition  and  slow  growth  during  this  period. 
We  found  that  assist  feeding  the  chick  on  occasion  and  offering  it  a mixture  of  food  items  was  a good 
i blend  of  strategies.  Weights  were  recorded  at  the  beginning  and  end  of  each  shift  to  ascertain  what 
I balance  of  the  two  strategies  was  appropriate.  Veterinary  staff  guidance  was  invaluable  at  this  stage 
and  the  keepers,  as  is  their  nature,  were  committed  to  balancing  objective  reasoning  with  passionate 
care. 

! 

We  found  that  by  offering  the  Ibis  mix  and  chopped  smelt  in  a small  amount  of  water  with  mealworms, 
crickets  and  chopped  pinkies  in  an  elevated  bowl  not  easily  reached  by  its  cage  mate  worked  best. 
It  appeared  that  the  chick  was  more  inclined  to  roost  and  feed  off  the  ground.  We  tried  to  monitor 
consumption  and  found  that  when  it  was  offered  this  way  the  chick  would  eat  up  to  250g  [~8.8oz.] 
of  food,  without  assistance.  The  chick  continued  to  be  very  intermittently  assist-fed  to  maintain  its 


Animal  Keepers'  Forum,  Vol.  37  No,  4/5  199 


weight  but  since  Day  73  the  chick  has  been  eating  completely 
on  its  own.  It  now  maintains  a weight  averaging  around  1kg 
[2.21bs.].  The  next  step  for  the  keepers  is  to  integrate  it  with 
the  larger  flock  in  order  to  socialize  it  for  a possible  departure 
to  another  institution  in  the  future. 

Conclusions 

We  believe  that  we  successfully  hand-reared  and  weaned  a 
healthy  Sacred  ibis  chick  from  hatching,  a first  for  the  Toronto 
Zoo.  The  chick  did  develop  slower  according  to  data  stated  in 
the  literature  and  does  continue  to  weigh  slightly  less  than  the 
other  adults  in  our  collection.  However,  we  feel  that  this  has  to 
do  with  the  fact  that  our  data  is  compared  that  of  parent-raised 
chicks  rather  then  hand-raised  chicks.  The  supplemental  assist 
feedings  after  weaning  were  only  used  as  a means  to  maintain 
the  chick’s  weight  and  overall  health.  Our  real  focus  was  on 
the  development  of  its  feeding  behaviour  and  it  was  clear  that 
having  another  bird  around  in  lieu  of  a parent  was  a vital  factor 
in  our  chick  learning  to  eat  on  its  own. 

References 

Holland,  G.  2007.  Encyclopaedia  of  Aviculture.  Hancock  House,  Surrey,  B.C.,  Canada. 

Soni,  K.  C.  2008.  Study  on  the  population,  foraging,  roosting  and  breeding  activities  of 

the  Black  ibis  /Red  napped  ibis  {Pseudibis  papillosa)  inhabiting  the  arid  zone  of  Rajasthan. 

A thesis  submitted  for  the  Award  of  Ph.D.  degree  in  Zoology,  University,  Ajmer  Faculty 
of  Science,  India.  (URL:  www.ibisring.org/static/the_black_ibis_thesis.pdf) 

Feather  Facts 

Feathers  allow  the  miracle  of  flight,  and 
protect  and  insulate.  In  many  species,  feathers 
attract  the  opposite  sex,  and  something  about 
feathers  might  attract  particular  humans  to 
a particular  bird  as  well.  For  hundreds  upon 
hundreds  of  years,  the  beauty  of  feathers 
have  so  appealed  to  humans,  they  have  used 
them  on  clothing,  hats,  in  their  hair,  dangled 
them  from  their  ears,  as  well  as  used  them 
for  ceremonial  purposes.  Feathers  evolved  from  the  scales  of  reptiles  and  set  birds  apart 
from  all  other  animals.  Birds  are  the  only  animals  that  have  feathers.  Feathers  are  made 
of  keratin,  the  same  protein  found  in  hair  and  nails.  Although  an  adult  bird  will  typically 
replace  all  of  its  feathers  during  a molt,  the  loss  of  feathers  is  staggered,  often  over  several 
months,  so  the  bird  has  enough  feathers  for  flight  and  insulation.  A molt  is  usually  triggered 
by  the  change  in  day  legnth  or  may  occur  after  breeduing. 


mL- ' Tr 


Source:  Doctors  Foster  and  Smith,  Pet  Education.com  and  Holly  Nash,  DVM,  MS 


200  Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  VoL  37  No.  4/5 


Semen  Collection  on  Rockhopper  Penguins 
at  the  Indianapolis  Zoo 

By  Tammy  Root,  Senior  Aquarist,  and  Jenny  Waldoch,  DVM 
Contributors:  Karen  Imboden,  Orville  VanDame,  Jeanette  Floss,  DVM 
Indianapolis  Zoo,  Indianapolis,  IN 


Why  we  began  collecting? 

The  Indianapolis  Zoo’s  Rockhopper  penguin  {Eudyptes  crestatus)  population  is  aged  and  is  not  self- 
sustaining,  which  will  lead  to  an  empty  exhibit  within  a few  years.  New  captive  animals  are  difficult 
to  acquire  and  expensive  because  of  the  same  lack  of  reproduction  at  other  zoos.  Wild  collection  of 
eggs  is  prohibitive  due  to  declining  populations,  government  import  and  quarantine  regulations,  and 
expense  and  time  commitment  of  reaching  remote  breeding  sites. 

Because  of  the  above  position  statement,  we  wanted  to  see  if  we  could  increase  the  number  of  eggs 
laid  by  artificially  inseminating  birds.  The  first  logical  step  to  begin  the  process  is  collecting  viable 
semen.  We  also  wanted  to  assure  ourselves  that  our  males  were  producing. 

How  did  I train  Rockhopper  penguins  for  collection? 

Since  this  has  never  been  done  before  on  Rockhopper  penguins,  I had  to  gather  my  technique 
information  from  poultry.  Not  only  was  I able  to  find  several  pieces  of  information  on  the  web,  but 
I was  also  able  to  visit  the  poultry  unit  at  Purdue  University,  West  Lafayette,  IN  and  practice  semen 
collections  on  their  poultry  flock. 

In  order  to  start  the  training,  I had  to  consider  a couple  of  factors  about  the  birds.  For  starters,  I 
only  trained  the  birds  that  were  owned  by  Indianapolis  Zoo.  This  allowed  me  to  train  6.0,0,  one  of 
which  was  bom  in  1999  and  the  others  were  bom  in  1987.  I also  had  to  take  into  account  that  none 
of  these  birds  were  handled  on  a regular  basis.  Unless  a medical  issue  appeared,  the  only  time  these 
birds  were  handled  was  once  every  three  weeks  for  routine  weighing.  (However,  all  of  our  birds  are 
hand-fed  twice  a day.) 

The  training  process  was  developed  in  many  steps.  Below  you  will  find  the  protocol  that  I used: 

1 .  Training  for  collection  presentation  (milking) 

a.  Begin  by  acclimating  bird  to  human  touch 

i.  Throughout  the  day  and  during  feeds,  slowly  approach  male  bird,  pat  the  bird’s 
stomach  and  reward  with  a capelin  fish  or  a verbal  “good  job”. 

1 . If  the  bird  eats  outside  of  the  feed,  the  number  of  fish  eaten  needs  to  be 
added  to  the  daily  total  amount 

2.  After  the  bird  feels  comfortable  with  the  belly  “pats”,  the  keeper  can  then 
continue  with  belly  “mbs”  or  massages.  See  Figure  1 : Belly  Pats 

3.  After  the  bird  feels  comfortable  with  belly  mbs,  trainer  begins  massaging 
the  vent  area.  See  Figure  2:  Vent  Area  Massaging  with  Bird  Upright. 

4.  All  of  the  training  steps  are  labeled  in  the  training  logs.  Belly  pats  are  BP; 
belly  mbs  are  BR  and  vent  area  massages  are  VA. 

b.  Once  the  trainer  feels  that  they  can  pick  up  the  bird  without  too  much  of  a fight,  the 
trainer  picks  up  the  bird  and  brings  the  bird  to  the  penguin  sink  counter.  Ideally,  you 
re-training  the  bird  not  only  to  be  comfortable  with  one  massaging  the  vent  area,  but 
also  getting  the  bird  comfortable  standing  on  the  sink.  (A  towel  is  placed  on  the 
stainless  steel  sink  for  bird’s  comfort).  Trainer  always  rewards  bird  with  a 

verbal  “good  job”. 


Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  VoL  37  No.  4/5  201 


i.  The  trainer  picks  up  the  bird  with  both  hands  holding  the  wings  close  to  the  bird’s 
body. 

ii.  All  of  these  birds  have  been  picked  up  before  for  weigh-ins,  so  they  should  not  get 
stressed  out. 

iii.  If  the  bird  becomes  too  stressed  (the  trainer’s  judgement  is  required),  set  the  bird 
back  down  on  exhibit  and  begin  rubbing  the  bird’s  stomach. 

iv.  Trainer  begins  vent  massaging  the  bird,  increasing  the  length  of  time  massaging  is 
being  done.  (Bird  is  standing  up) 

V.  Throughout  the  training,  trainer  should  continually  reward  the  bird  with  verbal 
sayings,  all  positive. 

vi.  If  the  bird  becomes  too  stressed,  try  belly  rubbing  or  belly  patting  the  bird  to  try  and 
calm  him  down.  If  this  doesn’t  work,  discontinue  the  session  for  that  time, 
c.  Finally,  trainer  should  begin  acclimating  the  bird  to  having  him  lay  on  his  belly. 

i.  This  part  of  training  should  be  done  either  on  exhibit  or  on  the  sink  counter. 

ii.  Trainer  will  need  to  restrain  bird  in  this  position  by  placing  an  arm  over  the  backside 
of  the  bird.  Restraint  should  continue  as  long  as  the  bird  feels  comfortable. 

iii.  Next,  trainer  lifts  bird’s  tail  and  using  both  hands,  massages  vent  area  in  an  inward/ 
upward  motion,  gradually  exposing  the  cloaca.  See  Figure  3:  Stimulating 

for  Collection. 

2.  Training  for  Sample  Collection 

a.  Train  bird  for  sample  collection  position  as  described  above 

i.  Before  collection  is  obtained,  cloaca  area  should  be  cleaned  with  a dry  towel  or  dry 
sterile  gauze. 

b.  Bring  in  second  person,  the  “collector” 

i.  Trainer  begins  massaging  and  exposes  the  cloaca. 

ii.  Collector  collects  semen  in  a Icc  syringe.  Trainer  and  collector  collaborate  on  each 
bird  to  detennine  when  to  stop  collecting,  (dependent  on  behavior  of  bird,  sample 
size,  etc.) 

iii.  Bird  is  placed  back  into  holding  to  calm  down. 

iv.  Collected  sample  is  then  analyzed. 

c.  Once  the  bird  is  trained  for  this,  the  whole  positioning  of  the  bird  and  collecting  of  the 
semen  should  only  take  5-10  minutes  (Zoo  Biology  18:199-214  [1999]). 

3 . Rules  of  Training 

a.  Diet,  weights,  stress  levels,  and  training  status  were  monitored  throughout  the  breeding 
season  by  using  an  easy  to  read  chart.  The  chart  will  include  columns  for  bird  ID#, 
time,  weight,  stage  of  training,  total  daily  food  intake  and  comments. 

b.  Weights  were  taken  weekly  before  the  morning  feed. 

c.  Diet  was  recorded  daily 

- d.  Training  time,  stress  levels  and  training  status  was  recorded  after  each  session, 

e.  Collection  training  was  done  with  both  non-breeding  and  breeding  males, 
f Rewards  were  always  be  food  or  positive  vocal  commands. 

g.  If  a bird  becomes  too  stressed  out  during  any  stage  of  training,  the  training  will  stop 
and  take  a step  backwards. 

h.  “Collection”  took  place  on  the  penguin  sink  counter.  Due  to  the  temperature  difference 
from  the  sink  to  the  exhibit,  body  temperature  will  be  watched  closely.  If  a bird 
becomes  too  warm,  the  session  will  end. 


202  Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5 


Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5  203 


The  stars  of  the  study  were: 

R113  with  a date  of  birth  of  18  Dec  1999 
R175  with  a date  of  birth  of  13  Dec  1987 
R191  with  a date  of  birth  of  14  Dec  1987 
R219  with  a date  of  birth  of  15  Dec  1987 
R221  with  a date  of  birth  of  15  Dec  1987 
R230  with  a date  of  birth  of  16  Dec  1987 


Were  we  successful  in  our  collection  attempts? 

We  started  collecting  on  17  September  2004,  a couple  of  weeks  prior  to  breeding  season.  During 
this  time,  all  of  the  birds  were  calm  during  each  collection.  We  were  able  to  collect  a sample  out  of 
five  birds.  However,  we  ran  into  problems  with  temperatures  of  the  microscope  and  the  hot  plate. 
Because  of  this,  we  were  unsure  of  what  the  sample  was,  R1 13  did  not  give  us  a sample  during  this 
time. 

The  first  week  we  collected  semen  with  sperm  was  on  1 October  2004.  We  saw  this  from  R221  and 
R230.  On  7 October  2004,  we  placed  river  rocks  on  exhibit  for  nesting  material  to  officially  start 
breeding  season. 

On  1 5 October  2004,  we  collected  again  and  were  able  to  see  sperm  on  five  of  the  males.  We  didn’t 
see  sperm  on  R1 13,  the  youngest.  By  28  October  2004,  we  were  able  to  see  sperm  on  all  of  the  birds 
involved  in  the  study.  From  this  point  forward,  we  collected  weekly  until  we  stopped  seeing  sperm 
from  each  bird  two  weeks  in  a row.  We  were  able  to  collect  semen  with  sperms  from  2-9  weeks, 
depending  on  the  bird.  We  were  able  to  collect  semen  with  sperm  on  R113  for  two  weeks  and  on 
R175  for  nine  weeks.  We  saw  the  last  amount  of  sperm  from  one  bird,  R175,  on  17  December 
2004. 

We  successfully  collected  semen  from  all  six  birds  in  the  study.  Most  of  the  collections  occurred  on 
Fridays  at  1400  hours.  During  R175’s  last  three  collections,  he  milked  himself  He  seemed  to  get 
the  signals  as  soon  as  he  was  picked  up  and  set  on  the  sink  counter.  He  received  all  of  the  cues  and 
knew  exactly  what  to  do! 

Throughout  the  study  each  bird  was  monitored  for  their  behavior,  food  consumptions  and  weight. 
Each  bird  exhibited  normal  penguin  behavior. 

In  2005,  we  added  eight  more  male  Rockhoppers  to  the  study  using  the  same  techniques.  During 
this  time,  we  also  conditioned  nine  females  for  artificial  insemination.  To  condition  for  artificial 
insemination,  we  used  the  following  techniques: 

1 . Place  a towel  on  the  penguin  sink  counter  and  duct  tape  it  down.  This  will  keep  the  bird 
from  slipping. 

2.  Catch  the  female  and  hold  her  on  the  towel. . .allowing  her  to  get  used  to  staying  on  the  counter. 

If  the  female  becomes  too  aggressive  or  uncomfortable,  release  her  back  into  holding  or  on 
exhibit  until  she  calms  down.  If  the  female  is  comfortable  and  relaxed,  reward  with  positive 
verbal  commands. 

3.  Once  the  female  becomes  comfortable  with  #2,  hold  her  belly  down  and  expose  the  vent  (same 
technique  as  conditioning  for  male  semen  collection  as  in  above  protocol). 

a.  Wipe  vent  area  with  dry  sterile  gauze 

b.  Gloves  will  be  worn  by  both  the  collector  and  the  trainer  throughout  the 
entire  procedure. 

4.  Afi;er  she  feels  comfortable  with  step  #3,  stroke  under  the  vent  area  to  encourage  her  to 
release  any  fecal  material. 

5.  Add  more  pressure  to  the  vent  area  to  expose  the  moist  pink  fleshy  internal  region  of  the  cloaca. 

6.  Insert  the  sperm,  using  a syringe,  into  the  exposed  cloaca  and  release. 

7.  Place  the  female  back  in  holding  or  on  exhibit  after  the  procedure  is  complete. 


204  Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  VoL  37  No  A! 5 


Rules  of  Training 

1 . Diet,  weights,  stress  levels  and  training  status  will  be  monitored  and  recorded  throughout  the 
training  process  with  an  easy-to-read  chart.  Chart  will  include  bird  ID#,  time,  weight,  stage 
of  training,  total  daily  food  intake  and  comments. 

2.  Weights  will  be  taken  weekly  only  before  breeding  season.  Once  breeding  season  starts, 
weights  will  not  be  taken  to  eliminate  some  stress  on  the  bird. 

3.  Diet  will  be  recorded  daily 

4.  Training  will  be  done  with  both  non-breeding  and  breeding  females 

5.  Rewards  will  always  be  positive  vocal  commands. 

6.  If  a bird  becomes  too  stressed  out  during  any  stage  of  training,  the  training  will  stop  and 
take  a step  backwards. 

7.  “Insemination  and  training”  will  take  place  on  the  penguin  sink  counter.  Due  to  the 
temperature  difference  from  the  sink  to  the  exhibit,  body  temperature  will  be  watched  closely. 

If  a bird  becomes  too  warm,  the  session  will  end. 

8.  At  no  time  will  both  the  female  and  the  male  be  off  the  nest.  There  will  always  be 

somebody  sitting  on  the  nest. 

9.  Throughout  the  entire  procedure,  only  non-contaminant  sample  will  be  used. 

10.  If  at  any  time  during  training  and/or  procedure  the  females  seem  to  be  losing  their 
nesting  behavior,  we  will  stop  artificially  inseminating  that  female. 

When  we  added  the  females  to  the  study,  we  would  first  collect  from  all  of  the  males.  After  each 
sample  was  collected  and  evaluated  for  quality,  the  samples  would  be  stored  in  a Styrofoam®  box 
kept  between  90-95°F  [32.2-35°C].  After  all  of  the  collections  were  completed,  the  good  samples 
would  be  pooled  together  and  diluted  with  semen  extender.  This  would  ensure  that  all  females  would 
receive  an  equal  amount  inseminated. 

In  2008,  we  extended  our  research  to  include  five  Gentoo  Penguins  (Pygoscelis  papua)  for  semen 
collection.  We  added  these  birds  to  establish  a baseline.  We  couldn’t  find  any  research  that  was  being 
done  regarding  Gentoo  penguin  semen  collection.  For  Gentoos  we  trained  in  the  following  manner: 

1 . Training  for  collection  presentation  (milking) 

a.  Begin  by  acclimating  bird  to  human  touch 

i.  Working  with  birds  on  landing 

a.  Condition  birds  to  come  to  the  top  of  the  stairs 

b.  Acclimate  birds  to  become  comfortable  in  keepers’  “laps” 

(bird  o floor  with  head  near  keeper’s  body)  for  various 
lengths  of  time. 

c.  Acclimate  birds  to  vent  area  back  massages  with  tail  lifted. 

d.  Acclimate  birds  to  having  cloaca  squeezed  for  milk 

2.  Training  for  Sample  Collection 
a.  Collection  is  same  as  it  is  with  Rockhoppers,  but: 

i.  Collector  will  be  down  the  stairs  from  the  bird.  It  will  be 
very  difficult  to  lift  the  bird  so  that  both  the  collector  and 
the  trainer  can  be  on  the  same  level. 

Over  the  years,  we  have  seen  much  cleaner  samples  with  Gentoos  than  with  Rockhoppers.  We  have 
learned  that  the  Gentoos  produce  a lot  of  urates  prior  to  collection.  The  Rockhoppers  still  tend  to 
vary  quite  a bit  in  this  respect.  We  are  trying  to  perfect  our  massaging  technique  in  hopes  of  getting 
cleaner  samples. 

References 

Martin,  R.D.  Artificial  Insemination  of  Poultry,  www.bemalpublishing.com  (2004) 

Root,  Tammy.  2000.  Revised  King  Training  Protocol. 

Semen  Collection,  Characterization,  and  Cryopreservation  in  a Magellanic  Penguin 
{Spheniscus  magellanicus).  Zoo  Biology  18:199-214(1999) 


Animal  Keepers'  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5  205 


Setting  Up  a Picture  Perfect  Success 

By  Katie  Bagley  and  Christopher  Watkinson,  Keepers 
Bird  Dept.,  Zoo  Atlanta,  Atlanta,  GA 

Zoo  Atlanta’s  male  and  female  Southern  ground  hombills  (Bucorvus  leadbeateri)  have  made  quite 
a name  for  themselves  and  are  arguably  the  most  popular  birds  in  the  zoo.  Both  birds  are  hand- 
raised  and  imprinted  on  people  and  therefore  interact  with  the  majority  of  guests  who  stop  by  their 
exhibit.  Visitors  will  usually  find  Zazu  and  Gumby  parading  their  food  items  in  front  of  their  plate- 
glass  viewing  window  (to  the  dismay  of  the  faint-of-heart)  and  at  risk  of  being  anthropomorphic, 
it  appears  as  though  the  attention  encourages  them  more.  When  the  excitement  of  their  food  has 
worn  off,  these  two  birds  will  resort  to  banging  their  beaks  on  their  window  or  other  items  in  their  I, 
exhibit  to  demonstrate  more  impressive  displays.  Our  ground  hombills  are  crowd  pleasers  because 
they  are  very  interactive  and  intelligent  birds,  but  they  also  require  a regular  schedule  of  enrichment.  , 
Furthermore,  Zazu  (our  female  hombill)  tends  to  exhibit  aggressive  behaviors  towards  her  keepers, 
which  motivates  us  to  provide  her  with  activities  that  enable  us  to  interact  with  her  in  a positive 
way. 

Zoo  Atlanta  has  a strong  animal  painting  program  that  includes  primates,  program  animals,  elephants, 
and  rhinos.  The  Bird  Department  felt  that  by  training  our  two  ground  hombills  to  paint,  we  would  ; 
be  able  to  provide  them  with  an  engaging  form  of  enrichment.  At  the  same  time  it  would  allow  a 
positive  keeper  interaction  for  Zazu  and  provide  zoo  guests  with  a fun  and  educational  experience.  ■ 
This  experience  has  taught  us  many  things  in  regards  to  training,  and  has  increased  our  knowledge  of  i 
these  two  birds  as  individuals.  Our  successes  were  often  preceded  by  many  failed  attempts.  It  was 
Zazu  and  Gumby  that  showed  us  what  they  needed  to  be  successful. 

Getting  Started 

We  already  knew  that  Zazu  and  Gumby  liked  wax  worms  and  liked  using  their  powerful  beaks  to 
manipulate  objects.  These  two  observations  provided  the  building  blocks  for  starting  the  painting 
behavior.  We  had  seen  Onyx,  the  Pied  crow  (Corvus  albus)  at  Zoo  Atlanta’s  wildlife  theater,  paint  in  ; 
demonstrations  so  we  started  with  a similar  set-up  (with  some  minor  adjustments)  for  our  hombills. 
Our  ultimate  training  goal  was  to  shift  the  hombills  inside  their  holding  area  (which  they  were  already 
trained  to  do)  while  we  set  up  their  painting  materials  in  their  exhibit.  We  would  then  release  them 
from  their  holding  area  into  their  exhibit  so  they  could  paint.  Afterwards,  we  would  shift  them  back 
into  their  holding  area  while  we  removed  the  painting  materials.  When  we  originally  set  up  their  | 
painting  materials  we  propped  up  two  canvases  that  were  attached  to  plywood  boards  against  the 
perimeter  fence  of  their  exhibit.  We  provided  two  paint  bmshes  that  were  modified  so  that  the  birds 
could  not  carry  them  off  (though  it  would  probably  delight  zoo  guests  if  they  did  so).  Cables  were 
attached  to  the  handles  of  bmshes  and  the  other  end  of  the  cable  was  connected  to  a carabineer  (clip) 
that  was  then  attached  to  the  perimeter  fence.  We  pre-dipped  the  bmshes  in  paint  for  the  hombills. 

The  first  time  Zazu  and  Gumby  encountered  their  painting  materials  they  were  immediately  curious 
about  the  new  items  in  their  exhibit.  They  first  tried  to  destroy  the  canvases  with  their  beaks  but  they 
soon  realized  that  they  were  rewarded  with  wax  worms  every  time  they  touched  the  paintbmsh  with 
their  beak.  From  this  point  we  jackpotted  them  every  time  they  held  the  paintbmsh  in  their  beaks  and 
rewarded  them  even  more  when  they  shook  the  bmsh  in  a stroking  motion.  The  birds  had  established 
the  behavior  of  picking  up  the  paint  bmshes  within  two  training  sessions.  We  initially  attached  the 
carabineer  low  on  the  fence  allowing  the  bmsh  to  lay  flat  on  the  ground.  While  the  hombills  could 
freely  manipulate  the  bmshes  they  were  having  a difficult  time  physically  connecting  the  bmsh  to  the 
canvas.  We  believe  this  was  due  to  the  way  the  bmshes  were  attached  to  the  fence.  It  was  apparent 


206  Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5 


eu<CwZHi-iZO 


Animal  Keepers'  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5  207 


that  we  had  to  change  this  set-up  because  Zazu  and  Gumby  seemed  to  be  getting  frustrated  and/or 
bored  with  the  sessions  since  they  would  drop  everything  and  go  back  to  parading  in  front  of  guests. 
Since  these  birds  were  physically  unique  compared  to  other  animals  we  had  observed  painting,  we 
decided  to  use  the  behavior  the  hombills  had  demonstrated  in  our  training  sessions  to  modify  the 
painting  set-up  and  to  set  them  up  for  success. 

Setting  Up  for  Success 

Based  on  our  earlier  training  sessions,  we  decided  to  set  up  their  painting  materials  in  a way  that 
made  it  almost  impossible  for  the  birds  to  fail.  We  first  attached  the  carabineers  higher  on  the  fence 
so  that  the  brushes  were  upright  and  level  with  the  canvases.  When  they  were  still  failing  to  make 
contact  with  the  brushes  we  placed  the  canvases  flat  on  the  ground  instead  of  placing  them  upright. 
The  brushes  were  making  contact  with  the  canvas  when  the  hombills  dropped  the  bmshes  but  they 
were  not  completing  the  desired  behavior.  Therefore,  we  designed  what  we  jokingly  referred  to  as 
“training  wheels.”  These  were  “L”  shaped  brackets  made  of  scrap  wood  with  a hole  drilled  out  on 
one  end.  A paintbmsh  was  then  inserted  upright  into  each  bracket  so  that  the  bmsh  end  was  in  contact 
with  the  canvas.  The  hole  in  the  bracket  was  large  enough  so  that  the  hombills  could  still  make  a 
stroking  motion  on  the  canvas  with  the  bmsh.  This  set-up  enabled  the  birds  to  simply  manipulate  the 
bmshes  slightly  with  their  beak  in  order  to  make  bmsh  strokes. 

Zazu  and  Gumby  quickly  learned  that  the  more  they  moved  the  bmsh  with  their  beak  the  more  wax 
worms  they  received.  We  also  believe  that  the  paint  colors  were  also  enriching  in  themselves  and  we 
soon  discovered  that  they  had  paint  colors  that  they  preferred  (Gumby  especially  loved  blue).  While 
the  hombills  became  fmstrated  and  would  leave  the  previous  set-up  after  lack  of  reinforcement, 
the  new  set-up  that  was  designed  for  them  as  individuals  proved  to  be  successful  in  that  they  could 
perform  the  behavior  when  cued  thus  receiving  food  reinforcement  often.  We  can’t  help  but  think 
that  the  positive  response  from  the  crowd  of  guests  watching  was  additional  reinforcement  for  Zazu 
and  Gumby.  With  a constant  stream  of  reinforcement  the  birds  remained  at  the  painting  stations  until 
cued  to  shift  inside  their  holding  so  we  could  take  the  canvases  out  of  the  exhibit.  Sometimes  the 
birds  would  continue  to  paint  without  food  reinforcement  during  our  failed  attempts  at  trying  to  shift 
them  while  the  crowd  cheered  them  on. 

Continuing  to  Make  Progress 

Our  current  goal  is  to  train  the  hombills  to  make  bmsh  strokes  without  the  use  of  their  “training 
wheels.”  To  further  help  Zazu  and  Gumby  be  successful  we  have  added  a horizontal  bar  to  the 
paintbmshes  to  give  them  multiple  options  in  holding  the  paintbmsh.  Zazu  seems  to  prefer  the 
horizontal  bar  while  Gumby  still  manipulates  the  actual  bmsh.  The  hombills  are  making  contact  with 
the  canvases  using  the  paintbmshes  but  they  are  still  learning  how  to  make  bmsh  strokes  without  the 
brackets.  We  are  currently  observing  their  behavior  with  the  bmshes  so  we  can  develop  ways  to  help 
them  be  successful. 

We  may  have  taught  Zazu  and  Gumby  how  to  paint  but  these  charismatic  birds  have  encouraged  us  to 
look  outside  the  box  and  analyze  their  individual  needs.  This  idea  can  be  applied  to  many  behaviors 
and  species.  Furthermore,  the  birds  now  have  an  activity  that  is  stimulating  and  enriching  while  zoo 
guests  can  watch  their  favorite  Southern  ground  hombills  show  off  their  talents.  As  keepers,  we 
are  enjoying  interacting  with  Zazu  without  aggressive  behaviors  being  exhibited  towards  us.  This 
opportunity  has  opened  many  doors  for  us  in  regards  to  our  avian  training  program  and  we  continue 
to  look  for  ways  to  set  all  of  our  birds  up  for  success. 


208  Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5 


Supplemental  Feedings  to  Aid  a Parent-Reared 
African  Pygmy-Falcon  (Polihierax  semitorquatus)  Chick 


By  Daniel  Boritt,  Biologist 
Department  of  Ornithology 

Smithsonian's  National  Zoological  Park,  Washington,  DC 

At  the  Smithsonian’s  National  Zoological  Park  we  have  had  recent  success  breeding  African  pygmy- 
falcons  {Polihierax  semitorquatus).  While  the  population  has  seen  a recent  surge  in  hatchings,  overall 
this  population  struggles  to  reach  its  target  levels  of  reproduction.  While  many  zoos  have  attempted 
to  breed  these  birds  in  off-exhibit  holding  and  hand-raise  the  chicks,  we  went  an  alternate  route  and 
bred  the  birds  on  exhibit  with  as  little  human  interaction  as  possible. 

Our  sire  is  currently  14  years  old,  and  was  unsuccessful  as  a breeder  until  early  2008  when  he  was 
already  12+  years  of  age.  In  late  2006/early  2007  this  bird  was  doing  quite  poorly  and  we  worried 
about  his  well-being.  Beginning  with  the  death  of  his  previous  exhibit  mate,  he  began  to  spend  more 
time  on  the  ground,  and  less  time  preening  himself  His  feather  condition  rapidly  deteriorated  as  did 
his  physical  abilities  to  fly.  While  he  has  never  been  a strong  flyer,  he  historically  had  been  able  to 
navigate  his  enclosure  with  ease,  but  as  his  condition  deterioriated  so  did  his  ability  to  move  around 
and  he  spent  a fair  amount  of  time  off  exhibit  under  veterinary  care  where  he  received  calcium  and 
vitamin  injections. 

In  early  2007  a new  piece  of  exhibit  furniture  was  added  to  the  enclosure.  An  2.43m  (8’)  tall  pine 
tree  with  numerous  horizontal  branches  was  cut  down  and  placed  into  the  exhibit.  The  horizontal 
perches  were  trimmed  to  create  something  akin  to  a spiral  staircase,  allowing  our  male  falcon  to  gain 
elevation  through  a series  of  short  hops  from  branch  to  branch.  This  simple  exhibit  modification 
had  immeadiate  and  drastic  benefits  for  this  bird.  He  soon  began  getting  off  the  ground  to  higher 
elevations  and  his  feather  condition  began  to  improve.  While  still  somewhat  lethargic  and  still  not 
particularly  active,  the  keeper  staff  began  to  gain  an  increased  level  of  comfort  with  his  condition. 

In  May  of 2007  we  received  an  eight-year-old  female  from  the  Franklin  Park  Zoo.  This  female,  being 
hand-reared,  had  a very  different  disposition  than  did  our  parent-reared,  older  male.  She  was  quite 
I mobile  and  also  quite  bold,  whereas  the  male  was  quite  timid  and  would  retreat  from  keeper  staff 

I upon  their  entering  the  enclosure.  The  new  female  on  the  other  hand  would  eagerly  approach  keeper 

I staff,  and  vocalize  and  head  bob  to  solicit  food.  The  birds  were  introduced  in  basement  holding 

I where  we  had  more  control  over  separating  the  birds,  and  within  ten  days  were  on  exhibit  in  the 

male’s  previous  enclosure. 

With  the  addition  of  the  female  the  older  male’s  condition  continued  to  improve.  He  became  more 
attentative  to  his  feathers,  preening  more  often,  and  spent  increasingly  more  time  on  the  higher 
elevation  perches,  often  perched  next  to  the  female.  In  June  2007  a naturalistic  nest  box  was  placed 
into  the  enclosure,  mounted  in  the  back  right  comer  of  the  exhibit  173cm  (5 ’8”)  off  the  ground,  just 
below  the  highest  horizontal  perches  on  the  aforementioned  pine  tree.  The  nest  box  is  a 5 1cm  (r8”) 
long  piece  of  maple  with  a circumference  of  41cm  (r4”)  at  its  thickest  point.  This  log  has  been  cut 
I in  half  vertically  and  a nest  chamber  carved  out,  the  cavity  was  left  very  rough  so  that  a chick  or 
parent  could  potentially  gain  purchase  to  aid  in  their  exit  from  the  nest.  The  log  was  then  resealed 
j and  any  gaps  filled  to  ensure  a secure  nest  log.  The  top  of  the  log  was  then  cut  off,  10cm  (4”)  from 

1 the  top,  and  fitted  with  a top  that  could  fit  securely  onto  the  log.  The  entire  nest  was  supported  by  a 

I steel  Z-wall  anchor  drilled  into  the  log,  that  slipped  easily  into  a bracket  mounted  on  the  exhibit  wall 
I for  seamless  removal.  The  nest  log  was  then  filled  first  with  Stalite®  (lightweight  aggregate)  (http:// 
WWW. stalite . com/)  and  the  pine  shavings  up  to  a level  approximately  8cm  (3”)  from  the  nest  cavity 
which  has  a diameter  of  6.5cm  (2.5”).  A dowel  was  inserted  into  the  nest  log  5cm  (2”)  below  the  nest 
cavity  for  easier  entrance  and  the  apparatus  was  mounted  on  the  wall. 


Animal  Keepers'  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5  209 


It  did  not  take  long  for  the  falcons  to  begin  exploring  the  nest  log,  with  both  male  and  female  making 
frequent  flights  to  the  top  of  the  dowel  and  eventually  exploring  the  cavity.  The  hen  laid  her  first  pair 
of  eggs  in  August  of 2007,  one  being  fertile  but  dying  prior  to  hatching,  while  the  second  was  infertile 
based  on  candling.  The  hen  laid  another  single  egg  in  October  of  the  same  year,  but  this  egg  too  was 
infertile.  In  late  November  the  pair  began  exploring  the  nest  log  and  spending  extended  periods  of 
time  inside.  By  2 1 December  2007  the  falcons  were  spending  extended  periods  of  time  inside  of  the 
nest  log.  At  this  point  we  began  to  limit  our  disturbances  of  the  exhibit,  only  performing  minimal 
cleaning,  and  entering  only  to  feed  the  birds.  By  29  December  2007  we  were  fairly  confident  the 
birds  had  laid  eggs  so  we  scheduled  a nest  inspection  and  on  4 January  2008  and  discovered  two 

African  pygmy-falcon  eggs,  one  of  which  was  determined  to 
be  fertile  by  candling. 

Although  this  female  had  successfully  reproduced,  due  to 
parental  neglect  keepers  at  Franklin  Park  Zoo  had  been  forced 
to  hand-rear  the  birds  in  order  to  ensure  survival.  With  this 
history  of  neglect  in  mind  we  formulated  a protocol  that 
would  balance  the  chick’s  survival  with  our  hopes  of  fledging 
a non-imprinted  chick.  With  the  aid  of  Karen  Lisi  in  the 
Zoo’s  Nutrition  Department  we  developed  a diet  as  well 
as  supplemental  feedings  of  the  chick  to  help  keep  the  bird 
thriving  through  its  early  days. 

On  1 7 January  2008  we  confirmed  the  hatching  of  one  chick  that 
had  hatched  the  day  before,  though  we  did  not  intervene  until 
the  following  morning.  On  the  morning  of  the  1 8 January  2008 
we  entered  the  exhibit  with  one  fuzzy  cut  into  1mm  segments, 
soaked  in  unflavored  Pedialyte®  fhttp://pedialyte.com/default. 
aspx),  and  warmed  to  approximately  36°C  [98.6°F].  Wearing 
eye  protection  and  longs  sleeves,  two  keepers  removed  the  nest 
log  from  the  wall  and  immobilized  the  hen  by  covering  her 
with  a hat  to  prevent  her  persistent  attacks.  We  then  removed 
the  chick  and  weighed  the  bird,  inspected  the  crop  and 
proceeded  to  feed  several  segments  of  warm  fuzzy  with 
the  aid  of  blunt  tipped  tweezers.  The  first  several  days 
we  fed  mainly  organ  meat,  but  made  sure  to  offer  all 
parts  of  the  body  to  simulate  a full  prey  item  as  closely 
as  we  could.  On  18  January,  Day  3,  the  chick  weighed 
eight  grams  [0.28  oz.]  (see  Table  1).  This  procedure 
continued  every  morning  by  0900hrs  through  the  first 
11  days.  While  we  were  feeding  the  bird  daily,  we 
never  fed  more  than  a single  2.5  grams  of  food,  and  our 
ultimate  goal  was  to  keep  the  chick  hydrated,  hoping  the 
parents  would  do  the  feeding.  As  you  can  see  by  the 
growth  chart  (Figure  1),  the  feedings  we  offered  were 
merely  supplemental  and  the  parents  were  feeding  the 
bird  well. 

Daily  weights  and  feedings  supplemented  with  Pedialyte® 
continued  through  Day  12,  at  which  point  the  chick 
had  reached  28  grams  [0.98  oz.].  From  days  13-17  we 
continued  to  weigh  the  bird  and  if  the  bird  had  not  gained  three  grams  we  would  feed  (this  was  only 
needed  on  Day  17,  when  the  bird  was  only  up  in  weight  by  two  grams).  Day  17,  1 February  2008, 
was  also  the  last  day  of  daily  weights  as  we  were  now  comfortable  that  the  parents  were  performing 
an  adequate  job  of  nourishing  the  chick. 


210  Animal  Keepers'  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5 


Table  1 

Day 

! 

Date 

16  * Jan 

Weight  |g| 

n/a 

2 

17 -Jan 

n/a 

3 

18- Jan 

8 

4 

19  - Jan 

8 

5 

20 -Jan 

9 

6 

21  - Jan 

12 

7* 

22 -Jan 

15 

8^ 

23 -Jan 

17 

9 

24 -Jan 

18 

10=^= 

25 -Jan 

19 

11 

26  - Jan 

25 

12 

27 -Jan 

OC 

13 

28 -Jan 

30 

14=^ 

29 -Jan 

34 

15 

30  - Jan 

OO 

16 

31  - Jan 

41 

17 

1-Feb 

43 

* Piclure  taken  on  this  date 

Figure  1 


Animal  Keepers'  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5  211 


1 0-day-old  chick  receiving  supplemental 

feeding.  (Photo:  Mehgan  Murphy) 


The  falcon  chick  began  to  reeeive  headless 
mealwomis  and  legless  crickets  on  7 February 
2008,  Day  23  post-hatch.  Observations 
continued  regularly  on  the  exhibit  and  food 
consumption  (by  means  of  displacement)  was 
closely  monitored  with  the  goal  of  always  have 
some  portion  of  the  diet  remaining  at  the  time 
of  the  next  feeding  (2x  daily).  On  11  February 
2008,  Day  27,  the  chick  was  again  removed  from 
the  nest  and  weighed,  tipping  the  scales  at  63 
grams  (2.22  oz).  On  14  February  2008,  Day  30, 
the  chick  was  observed  peering  out  of  the  nest 
log  for  the  first  time.  On  18  February  2008,  Day 
34,  the  chick  finally  fledged  from  the  log,  though 
was  soon  back  inside  where  it  continued  to  spend  the  majority  of  its  time  until  early  March.  The 
chick  continued  to  live  on  exhibit  with  the  parents  until  27  June 
2008,  when  we  determined  that  parental  aggression  had  reached 
a concerning  level.  The  chick  was  removed  and  weighed  60 
grams  (2.12  oz),  three  grams  (0. 1 1 oz)  less  than  our  last  recorded 
weight  on  1 1 February  2008. 


16-day-old  chick  receiving  supplemental  feeding 

on  scale.  (Photo:  Mehgan  Murphy) 


Over  the  next  two  years  we  had  numerous  clutches  of  eggs  laid 
by  the  pair  with  limited  fertility  and  survivability  (all  fertile  eggs 
were  early  dead).  We  continued  to  occasionally  offer  the  nest  log 
but  had  little  faith  we’d  get  another  chick  due  to  the  male’s  age 
and  history  of  medical  problems.  However  on  25  June  2009,  we 
were  yet  again  pleased  to  find  one  African  pygmy-falcon  chick 
in  the  very  same  nest  log.  The  chiek  is  still  on  exhibit  with  its 
parents  and  doing  well. 


25-day-old  chick  about  to  fledge 
peers  tentatively  out  of  nestlog. 

(Photo:  Dan  Boritt) 


212  Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5 


Breeding  White-crested  Laughing  Thrush 

(Garrulax  leucolophus)  at  the  Birmingham  Zoo 


By  Alan  Tester,  Bird  Keeper;  Jejf  Fribble,  Zoological  Manager  of  Birds; 
and  Cindy  Finger,  Curator  of  Birds,  Birmingham  Zoo,  Birmingham,  AL 


Introduction 

The  Birmingham  Zoo  has  successfully  raised  White-crested  Laughing  Thrush  {Garrulax  leucolophu) 
s from  2006  through  2009,  by  both  parent-rearing  and  hand-rearing.  These  active,  vocal  birds  are 
a good  exhibit  species  and  a compatible  pair  will  breed  in  a moderate  sized  exhibit  with  a basic, 
functional  habitat.  Mortality  was  high  in  both  hand  and  parent  reared  chicks,  6 1 % total.  We  identified 
two  problems  responsible  in  part  for  the  chick  mortalities  that  we  were  able  to  correct  or  treat.  We 
experimented  with  three  hand-rearing  diets  and  had  success  with  each.  Parent-rearing  was  successful 
once  the  exhibit  was  rat  proofed. 

Biology/Habits 

The  White-crested  laughing  thrush  is  native  to  the  southern  Himalayas  and  southeast  region  of  Asia 
where  it  inhabits  mountain  forests,  including  bamboo  stands.  Males  and  females  of  the  species  are 
identical  in  size,  15-20cm  (6-8  in.)  tall,  weight  112-120g  [3.95-4.23  oz.],  and  in  coloration,  both 
sexes  having  a white  underside  and  white  crest,  striking  contrasts  to  the  brown  upper-side  and  black 

mask. 

White-crested  laughing  thrushes  are  active,  vocal  birds  making  them  an  excellent  exhibit  species. 
Our  individuals  are  not  bothered  by  the  public,  remaining  inquisitive  even  when  being  admired  by  a 
group  of  visitors  Birds  spend  almost  as  much  time  foraging  on  the  ground  as  they  do  hopping  from 
perch  to  perch.  Laughing  thrushes  mutter  and  fuss  while  they  go  about  their  daily  business,  but  these 
casual  vocalizations  are  calm  compared  to  their  boisterous  laughing.  Males  and  females  perform 
loud  duets,  which  each  sex  singing  a distinct  part. 

I The  diet  of  wild  laughing  thrushes  includes  insects  and  fruit  items.  The  Birmingham  Zoo  diet  is 
Apple  Paradise®  pellets  with  chopped  fruit  and  vegetables,  meat  (about  5g/0. 1 7 oz.)  and  mealworms. 

! Crickets  and  wax  worms  are  tossed  into  the  exhibit  in  the  afternoon. 

I 

i Exhibit  Design 

j The  Birmingham  Zoo’s  laughing  thrush  exhibit  is  3m  x 2.4m  x 3.7m  (12  ft.  x 8 ft.  x 10  ft.).  The  upper 
half  of  the  exhibit  is  currently  covered  with  1.37  cm  (.5  in)  hardware  cloth.  Three  sides  of  the  lower 
half  are  solid  concrete.  During  the  breeding  seasons  of  2006  through  2009  the  bottom  half  of  the 
exhibit  front  was  glass,  but  has  recently  been  replaced  with  mesh.  The  birds  are  cold  tolerant  and  the 
mild  Alabama  winters  allow  us  to  keep  the  birds  on  exhibit  year  round  with  no  indoor  access.  A heat 
lamp  is  provided  in  winter  but  is  rarely  used.  An  overhang  in  the  back  of  the  exhibit  provides  shelter 
from  rain  and  a grove  of  bamboo  in  front  of  the  exhibit  provides  shade  in  summer  afternoons. 

I 

The  exhibit  is  planted  with  two,  seven-foot  high  evergreen  trees  which  provide  upper-story  cover  and 
! nest  sites.  Dwarf  evergreens  and  grasses  provide  ground  cover.  The  exhibit  floor  is  concrete  covered 
I with  a thick  layer  of  soil  and  a thin  layer  of  hardwood  mulch.  Tree  limbs  provide  natural  looking 
j perching  and  a small,  shallow  pool  provides  water  for  drinking  and  bathing. 

I 

We  provide  our  pair  with  a ready  made  nest,  a wicker  basket  with  a 15cm  (6  in.)  diameter  and  a 10 
to  15cm  (4  -6  in.)  cup  depth.  The  bottom  of  the  basket  is  thinly  covered  with  hardwood  mulch.  Our 
birds  never  add  much  material  to  the  nest,  but  do  loosely  line  the  bottom  and  sides  with  grasses  and 
leaves  they  find  in  the  exhibit.  Nesting  material,  such  as  palm  fibers,  feathers  and  dry  bamboo  leaves 

I 

i Animal  Keepers'  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5  213 


have  been  offered.  Only  the  bamboo  leaves  have  been  used  and  these  often  fall  into  the  exhibit  on ' 
their  own.  The  nest  is  placed  in  an  evergreen  tree  1.8  to  2.1m  (6  ft.  to  7 ft.)  above  the  ground.  In 
2007,  the  male  and  his  new  mate  started  to  build  a nest  with  small  sticks  about  a foot  away  from  the 
nest  basket  located  in  the  same  spot  the  previous  female  had  nested.  The  basket  was  moved  to  the 
spot  they  were  building  their  nest  and  they  immediately  started  using  the  basket. 

The  past  breeding  season  our  laughing  thrush  pair  shared  the  exhibit  with  a pair  of  Bamboo  Partridges 
(Bambusicola  fytchii  fytchii).  There  was  no  aggression  between  the  adults,  but  the  laughing  thrushes 
killed  all  the  partridge  chicks  that  hatched  on  exhibit.  We  have  never  housed  laughing  thrushes  with 
any  other  species. 

Breeding  Overview 
2006-2009 

The  Birmingham  Zoo  has 
successfully  bred  White- 
crested  laughing  thrushes 
from  2006  through  2009, 
having  raised  nine  chicks 
total,  four  hand-reared  and 
five  parent-reared.  The 
same  male  has  bred  with 
two  females.  The  breeding 
season  in  Alabama  is  March 
through  August,  with  the 
earliest  egg  laid  23  March 
and  the  latest  egg  laid  on  18 
July.  The  first  clutch  is 
typically  laid  mid  April  to 
early  May,  with  three  to  four 
clutches  possible  depending 
on  success  of  early  clutches 
and  if  chicks  are  pulled  for 
hand-rearing.  Our  laughing  thrushes  typically  lay  three  to  four  eggs  and  no  fewer  than  two.  The 
parents  share  incubation  duties  beginning  with  the  first  egg,  so  chicks  hatch  asynchronously.  As 
many  as  four  chicks  have  hatched  and  once  all  four  fledged,  but  we  have  never  had  more  than  two 
chicks  in  the  same  clutch  survive.  All  the  laughing  thrush  chicks  at  our  zoo  have  fledged  at  seven  to 
12  days  old  and  while  they  look  like  little  laughing  thrushes  at  this  stage  they  are  unable  to  fly.  The 
chicks  look  and  behave  more  like  branchers  than  fledglings  but  the  chicks  consistently  leave  the  nest 
at  around  10  days.  Even  when  the  birds  are  placed  back  in  the  nest  they  soon  hop  out. 

Our  laughing  thrush  pair  tolerates  periodic  nest  checks.  They  fuss  and  scold,  but  always  return  to 
the  nest  or  continue  to  feed  chicks  after  the  keeper  has  either  visually  peeked  or  gently  felt  into  the 
nest.  We  avoid  up  close  nest  inspections  starting  eight  days  after  the  first  chick  hatches  to  avoid 
spooking  the  chicks  into  fledging  earlier  than  they  already  do.  We  are  usually  able  to  count  heads 
from  outside  the  exhibit  at  this  stage  and  do  not  need  to  be  intrusive.  The  female,  hand-raised,  has 
attacked  keepers  but  not  during  nest  checks. 

Monitoring  nests  is  a helpful  management  tool,  though  may  not  be  possible  with  every  pair  or  in 
every  exhibit  depending  on  the  nest  location.  Checking  eggs  and  chicks  every  few  days  provides 
important  information,  such  as  how  many  eggs  are  laid  and  how  many  chicks  hatch.  Keepers  use 
this  information  to  recognize  problems,  whether  in  the  egg  (infertility,  poor  incubation  etc.),  or  in  the 
early  stages  of  development  (lack  of  food,  environmental  factors  etc.) 


Birmingham  Zoo’s 
Laughing  Thrush  Exhibit. 


214  Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5 


The  following  is  a breeding  summary  for  our  laughing  thrushes. 

2006:  Our  original  pair  laid  four  clutches.  The  two  chicks  from  the  first  clutch  were  found  dead  on 
the  ground  at  Day  2,  the  second  and  third  clutches  fledged  but  were  lost  to  rat  predation.  The  fourth 
clutch  hatched  in  late  July,  soon  after  which  the  female  was  found  dead.  The  male  raised  two  chicks 
by  himself  until  they  fledged  at  12  days  of  age.  One  of  the  chicks  was  observed  gasping  and  both 
were  pulled  for  hand-rearing.  The  gasping  chick  died  within  24  hours  as  the  result  of  a wasp  lodged 
deep  in  its  throat,  not  discovered  until  the  necropsy.  Presumably  the  male  tried  to  feed  the  wasp  to 
the  chick  and  it  did  not  go  down  well. 

2007:  A new  female  was  suecessfully  introduced  to  our  male  in  an  off-exhibit  holding  area  where 
they  could  first  be  “howdied”  next  to  each  other.  Two  eggs  in  the  first  clutch  disappeared  five  days 
later.  The  second  clutch  produced  four  chicks.  At  Day  10  one  chick  was  found  dead  on  the  ground 
partially  eaten.  The  remaining  three  chicks  were  pulled  for  hand-rearing  to  avoid  further  predation. 
We  successfully  raised  one  chick  from  this  clutch.  The  second  clutch  we  planned  on  leaving  with  the 
parents,  but  pulled  them  after  the  female  had  to  be  removed  due  to  aggression  by  the  male. 

2008:  Two  clutches  were  laid,  the  first  in  early  June  the  second  in  mid- July.  Two  eggs  from  the 
first  clutch  were  found  in  the  pool,  the  third  egg  hatched  and  the  chick  was  hand  raised.  The  second 
clutch  contained  four  eggs,  one  disappeared  and  three  hatched.  All  three  chicks  fledged  with  two 
surviving. 

2009:  The  first  clutch,  three  eggs,  was  laid  in  early  May.  One  egg  was  infertile,  one  egg  disappeared 
and  one  chick  hatched  and  survived.  All  four  chicks  from  the  second  clutch  fledged  and  two 
survived. 

Hand-rearing 

The  Birmingham  Zoo  has  attempted  to  hand-raise  nine  chicks  (none  younger  than  five  days)  and 
successfully  reared  four  (44%).  The  one  chick  died  from  the  wasp,  one  ehick  was  in  a very  weak 
condition  when  pulled  and  survived  only  half  a day,  one  died  within  three  days  of  unknown  causes 
and  the  other  three  died  from  aspergillosis.  The  first  three  successes  were  the  strongest  of  their 
respective  clutches  and  the  fourth  was  an  only  chick. 

Chicks  were  raised  in  a 91cm  x 51cm  x 51cm  (36  in.  x 20  in.  x 20  in.)  wire  cage  with  5cm  x 2cm  (2 
I in.  X 0.25  in.)  gaps,  with  perches  at  various  levels  to  make  it  easy  for  them  to  get  around.  The  bottom 
of  the  cage  was  lined  with  newspaper.  Two  small  doors  allow  keeper  access  for  feeding  and  cleaning. 
All  but  one  chick  we  pulled  were  feathered  and  did  not  need  an  external  heat  souree,  though  we 
‘ provided  a heat  lamp  for  one  clutch  because  two  of  the  chicks  appeared  weak. 

j We  altered  the  chick  diet  each  of  the  three  years  we  hand  raised  laughing  thrushes  trying  to  find  the 
diet  that  promoted  proper  chiek  development  while  being  easy  to  prepare  and  feed  to  the  chicks, 
j The  first  year  we  were  not  expecting  to  pull  the  chieks.  The  two  chicks  were  nine  and  10  days  old 
j when  we  pulled  them  (wts.  46g  and  45g  / 1.62  oz.  and  1.58  oz.)  and  had  a good  feeding  response  to 
I tweezers,  including  the  one  with  a wasp  lodged  in  its  throat  (chick  died  the  next  day).  We  followed 
I an  hourly  feeding  schedule,  from  0800  to  1700  hrs,  until  the  surviving  chick  was  weaned.  We 
offered  food  until  the  chick  stopped  begging,  feeding  a combination  of  mealworms,  crickets  and 
I soaked  Apple  Paradise®  pellets  (20%  protein,  7 % fat)  the  first  1 1 days  of  hand-rearing.  Wax  worms, 
while  digestible  by  adults,  are  not  digestible  by  the  chicks;  even  14-day-old  chicks  passed  whole 
wax  worms.  The  pellets  were  soaked  to  provide  liquid  and  were  not  offered  at  every  feeding.  We 
squished  the  heads  on  the  insects  before  feeding  them  to  chicks  to  prevent  the  insects  from  biting  the 
birds  as  they  were  swallowed.  At  age  22  days  we  fed  the  chick  pinkie  miee  cut  into  fourths.  The 
chick  readily  accepted  the  pinky  pieces  and  consumed  1 to  1.5  every  hour.  Our  pinky  supply  was 
low  so  at  Day  25  we  returned  to  feeding  crickets  and  mealworms.  We  started  to  wean  the  chick  at  29 


Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5  215 


days,  forcing  her  to  grab  the  food  instead  of  pushing  the  food  into  her  mouth,  feeding  only  what  she 
took  from  the  tweezers.  The  chick  was  eating  out  of  a food  bowl  at  3 1 days  and  was  entirely  self- 
feeding at  32  days  (wt.  70g/2.47  oz.),  though  she  still  begged  for  food. 

In  2007  we  pulled  two  broods  of  three  ehieks  eaeh  for  hand-rearing  as  soon  as  the  ehicks  fledged. 
The  first  brood  was  pulled  in  late  June,  the  ehieks  were  about  7-10  days  old  (wts.  53g,  49g  and 
48g).  We  initially  tweezer-fed  them  inseets  and  soaked  Apple  Paradise®  pellets  which  they  readily 
aeeepted.  During  this  period  their  weights  decreased  (43g,  41g  and  39g).  The  smallest  died  on  Day 
4.  A second  chick  died  on  Day  7 from  aspergillosis.  The  fifth  day  of  hand-rearing  we  changed  to 
syringe  feeding  in  addition  to  mealworms.  We  soaked  Apple  Paradise®  pellets  in  water  until  all  the 
liquid  was  absorbed  then  blended  the  pellets  with  additional  water  and  a little  calcium  to  make  a 
“smoothie.”  The  remaining  ehiek’s  weight  inereased  sufficiently  after  three  days  of  syringe  feedings 
that  we  returned  to  tweezer  feeding  mealworms  and  the  regular  adult  diet  of  pellets  and  chopped 
fruits  and  vegetables  with  only  one  or  two  syringe  feedings  a day.  The  ehick  was  self-feeding  on 
mealworms  at  Day  24  and  eompletely  self-feeding  at  27  days  (wt  69g). 


The  ehieks  from  the  second  brood  were 
also  ages  7 to  10  days  (wts.  50g,  44g 
and  39g)  when  pulled.  The  first  three 
days  we  syringe  fed  each  chick  l-3cc  of 
soaked  pellets  every  half  hour  hoping 
to  avoid  the  weight  deerease  we  had  the 
time  before;  however,  each  chick  lost 
weight.  The  smallest  ehick  was  weak 
and  died  the  next  day.  A second  chick 
died  the  following  day;  its  weight  was 
39g.  The  third  chiek  was  back  to  50g 
after  four  days  of  syringe  feeding  with 
10-12  ce  a day.  At  age  19  days  we 
tweezer-fed  its  regular  diet  and  the  bird 
was  self-feeding  at  age  27  days  (wt.  75g). 


Recently  Fledged  Laughing  Thrush  Chiek 


We  planned  to  let  the  parents  rear  all  their  ehieks  in  2008,  but  pulled  the  only  chick  from  the  first 
brood,  hatehed  17  June,  after  it  was  found  on  the  ground  beneath  the  nest  at  five  days  (wt.  28g).  The 
ehiek  was  eold  and  was  plaeed  in  a elosed  brooder  box  set  at  a temperature  of  34°C  (93 °F).  We  used 
Kaytee  Exact®  hand  feeding  fonuula  (22%  protein,  9%  fat),  mixed  according  to  directions,  in  place 
of  the  Apple  Paradise®  smoothie  as  an  experiment.  After  initial  feedings  of  0.5  cc  of  formula  every 
half  hour,  with  additional  water  to  inerease  hydration,  we  fed  the  ehick  1 .5  to  2.5cc  formula  every 
hour  for  the  first  day.  The  ehick  gained  4g,  more  than  the  target  10%  weight  gain.  The  second  day 
we  fed  2-4ee  of  formula  every  1.5  hours.  The  third  day,  age  seven  days,  we  began  feeding  the  chick 
pinky  mice  eut  into  thirds.  The  pinkies  were  warmed  and  sprinkled  with  ealcium  before  feeding.  At 
age  nine  days  we  moved  the  chiek  to  an  open  brooder  box  with  a heat  lamp  overhead  keeping  the 
temperature  at  26°C  (78  F)  but  found  the  ehiek  to  be  most  comfortable  at  24°C  (75°F).  From  age 
10  to  age  18  days  we  fed  half  pinkies  every  one  to  one  and  a half  hours,  as  many  eight  pieces  (four 
whole)  per  feeding  then  switehed  to  tweezer-feeding  insects,  pellets  and  fruit/vegetable  mix  from  the 
food  bowl  that  was  left  in  the  eage  during  the  day.  The  chick  was  self- feeding  at  Day  25  (wt.  81g). 


Evaluation  of  Hand-rearing  Methods 

We  suceessfully  hand-raised  ehieks  with  each  of  the  three  chick  diets:  insects  and  pellets,  insects 
and  liquefied  pellets  and  liquid  formula  and  pinkies.  A diet  of  insects  and  dry  pellets  worked  well, 
but  feeding  small  food  items  by  tweezers  ean  be  time  consuming.  Syringe  feeding  takes  less  time 
and  provides  hydration.  Chieks  responded  well  to  the  syringe  and  would  “swallow”  the  syringe, 
speeding  up  feeding  time  and  reducing  the  risk  of  getting  food  in  the  traehea.  Syringe  feeding  is 
espeeially  easier  than  tweezer-feeding  when  feeding  weak  ehicks.  The  pinkies  were  also  quick  to 


216  Animal  Keepers'  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5 


feed.  Weight  gain  was  not  significantly  better  on  pinkies  and  the  biggest  weight  gain  occurred  after 
the  switch  to  insects,  but  the  sample  size  is  much  too  small  to  say  this  is  significant  or  even  typical 

Pinkies  are  expensive  and  may  be  cost  prohibitive.  Mealworms  and  crickets  can  also  be  expensive 
and  at  times  are  in  short  supply.  A combination  of  liquefied  pellets  or  a powder  chick  formula  in 
combination  with  insects  is  perhaps  the  best  diet.  Less  time  is  spent  feeding  the  chicks,  assures 
proper  hydration  and  is  less  expensive.  Age,  weight  and  overall  condition  of  the  chicks  at  the  time 
they  are  pulled  should  be  taken  into  consideration  when  determining  what  to  feed  the  chicks.  Week- 
old  chicks  should  handle  pinkies  well  (Mace,  2007).  Laughing  thrush  chicks  can  be  weaned  by  27 
days  regardless  of  the  rearing  diet. 

Concerns 

Aspergillosis  was  a problem  each  time  we  hand-reared  chicks.  Three  chicks  died  from  aspergillosis 
and  three  other  chicks  were  successfully  treated.  The  survivors  were  treated  with  Itraconazole 
administered  orally  and  nebulized,  every  day  for  a week  with  a mixture  of  Enrofloxacin,  Itraconazole 
and  Amphotericin  B.  We  have  successfully  raised  flamingos,  pigeons,  lorikeets,  ducks  and  quail  in 
the  same  room  we  reared  our  laughing  thrushes  in,  and  only  had  a couple  of  aspergillosis  cases  in 
those  species. 

Imprinting  is  a possibility  and  could  be  a problem.  The  first  laughing  thrush  we  hand  raised  is 
severely  imprinted,  becoming  active  and  vocal  at  a mere  glimpse  of  a keeper  and  aggressively 
attacking  certain  keepers.  She  is  housed  with  a hand-raised  male  who  is  not  as  heavily  imprinted. 
This  pair  has  not  attempted  to  breed.  The  other  three  chicks  we  hand  raised  did  not  appear  to  be  as 
imprinted  as  the  first,  but  we  did  not  keep  them  longer  than  eight  months  so  we  do  not  know  their 
long  term  behavior.  We  tried  to  keep  talking  to  a minimum  around  the  chicks  and  handled  them 
only  when  necessary.  Hand-raised  birds  will  breed.  Our  current  breeding  female  was  hand  raised 
by  another  institution  and  behaves  in  relatively  the  same  manner  as  the  bird  we  raised,  being  very 
responsive  to  keepers.  She  has  attacked  keepers,  but  not  frequently.  Her  mate  is  parent-reared  and 
was  already  a proven  breeder. 

Parent  Rearing 

Our  White-crested  laughing  thrush  have  had  the  opportunity  to  rear  14  chicks  to  completion  (including 
the  four  predated),  five  survived.  The  first  two  breeding  seasons,  2006  and  2007,  we  provided  the 
I parents  with  freshly  molted  (white)  mealworms,  ones  whose  skin  had  not  hardened.  Freshly  molted 
' worms  were  offered  to  avoid  impaction  as  the  chitin  skins  may  be  hard  for  chicks  to  digest.  When 
I we  could  not  get  enough  freshly  mottled  worms  we  offered  crickets  with  the  hind  legs  removed. 
After  seven  days  we  started  offering  regular  mealworms  and  crickets.  Parents  are  meticulous  about 
masticating  the  heads  of  the  crickets  and  mealworms  before  filling  their  beaks  with  the  insects  and 
flying  to  the  nest  one  at  a time.  The  parents  always  feed  the  chicks  before  they  feed  themselves.  We 
provided  insects,  placed  in  a bowl  on  the  ground,  every  two  hours.  We  keep  the  exhibit  pool  drained 
; once  chicks  are  in  the  nest  to  avoid  fledglings  drowning  and  provide  water  in  a shallow  pan.  All  the 
I chicks  that  fledged  in  this  time  period  were  pulled  for  hand-rearing. 

Beginning  in  2008,  we  started  keeping  a constant  supply  of  mealworms  available  to  the  parents, 
regardless  of  molted  or  not.  We  thought  the  chicks  might  be  fledging  early  because  the  lack  of  food, 
i While  compaction  from  mealworms  may  be  possible,  we  decided  poor  condition  from  lack  of  food 
was  a greater  risk.  We  never  had  any  problems  with  chicks  digesting  mealworms.  Even  with  the 
I increased  supply  of  mealworms  no  more  than  two  chicks  per  brood  survived  and  chicks  still  fledged 
I at  about  10  days  of  age.  We  did  not  weigh  parent-raised  chicks  until  they  were  pulled  from  the 
j exhibit  so  we  have  no  comparison  to  hand  raised  weights.  Parent-reared  chicks  were  weaned  at  about 
I five  weeks,  a week  longer  than  we  were  able  to  wean  chicks  during  hand-rearing. 

! 

I Three  chicks  of  the  second  2008  clutch  fledged  and  two  were  successfully  parent-reared,  the  first 

Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5  217 

I 


time  for  our  zoo.  In  2009  one  chick 
hatched  in  late  May  and  survived.  We 
placed  the  bowl  of  worms  on  a small 
elevated  platform  the  bamboo  partridges 
were  unable  to  access,  otherwise  the 
partridges  were  apt  to  eat  many  of  the 
worms.  A second  clutch  was  laid  20 
days  after  the  first  chick  hatched.  All 
four  eggs  from  this  clutch  hatched.  One 
chick  was  found  dead  below  the  nest 
nine  days  after  hatching.  The  other  three 
chicks  were  all  fledged  four  days  later. 

Unlike  other  broods  that  all  fledged  on 
the  same  day,  one  chick  stayed  in  the  nest 
a full  day  longer  than  its  two  siblings. 

The  smallest  chick  was  found  dead  two  days  after  fledging.  The  other  two  survived. 

Helpers,  offspring  that  help  raise  younger  siblings,  have  been  observed  in  White-throated  Laughing 
Thrushes  (Garrulax  albogularis)  and  Yellow-throated  Laughing  Thrushes  {Garrulax  galbanus) 
(Mace,  2007).  The  chick  from  the  first  clutch  was  not  observed  feeding  the  younger  siblings  as  he 
was  only  recently  self-feeding  and  would  still  food  beg  (occasionally  getting  fed)  while  the  second 
brood  was  being  cared  for.  All  three  chicks  were  left  in  the  exhibit  with  the  parents  until  the  end  of 
August.  No  further  nesting  was  attempted. 

Evaluation  of  Parent  Rearing 

Parent-rearing  went  well  once  the  predation  problem  was  fixed,  with  63%  of  all  hatchlings  successfully 
raised.  Keeping  a constant  supply  of  worms  available  is  recommended,  but  all  the  chicks  may  still 
not  survive.  Asynchronous  hatching  may  be  a cause  of  the  high  chick  mortality.  Keepers  observed 
the  parents  feeding  all  chicks  in  a given  brood  but  as  happens  in  the  wild  the  strongest,  usually  the 
oldest,  may  get  the  most  food,  even  when  there  is  no  food  shortage.  A survey  of  captive  Blue-crowned 
Laughing  Thrush  {Dryonastes  courtois)  showed  a chick  mortality  of  79.8%  (Edmans,  2008). 

Concerns 

Our  primary  obstacle  to  successful  parent-rearing  was  chick  predation  by  rats.  The  first  two  years 
we  were  unable  to  keep  chicks  with  the  parents  past  fledging,  their  inability  to  fly  and  their  habit  of 
sleeping  on  the  ground  or  low  to  the  ground  made  them  vulnerable  to  rats.  At  that  time  the  exhibit 
was  covered  with  2.5  cm  x 5 cm  (1  in  x 2 in)  wire  mesh  which  rats  were  able  to  squeeze  through. 
Switching  to  1.3  cm  (0.5  in)  hardware  cloth  solved  the  problem. 

Post-nesting  aggression  occurred  twice.  The  male  became  aggressive  toward  his  mate  during  August 
2007  and  2008  soon  after  the  last  brood  of  the  year  fledged.  The  current  female  does  not  feed  the 
chicks  as  much  after  they  fledge,  though  she  is  very  attentive  when  they  are  in  the  nest.  If  the  female 
goes  back  on  the  nest  there  is  no  aggression,  only  after  the  last  brood  has  fledged  does  the  male 
become  aggressive  towards  her.  The  female  suffered  severe  trauma  to  the  back  of  the  neck  in  2007 
and  required  stitches.  The  pair  was  kept  separated  until  February  of  2008.  The  female  was  pulled 
as  soon  as  aggression  was  observed  in  August  2008.  We  put  the  pair  back  together  one  month  later 
without  further  incident.  The  aggression  may  be  due  to  1)  The  male  wants  the  female  to  return  to  the 
nest  or  2)  The  male  takes  exception  to  the  female  ignoring  the  chicks  after  they  fledge.  We  suspect 
the  first.  There  was  no  aggression  in  2009,  perhaps  because  two  clutches  were  successful.  We  have 
observed  this  aggressive  behavior  in  our  Victoria-crowned  Pigeon  {Goura  Victoria)  pair.  We  pull  the 
female  soon  after  the  chick  fledges  and  the  male  raises  the  chick  alone. 

We  had  a problem  in  2009  with  the  Bamboo  partridges  roosting  in  the  Laughing  thrush  nest.  The 


Adult  Laughing  Thrush  Feeding  Chick 


218  Animal  Keepers'  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5 


evergreen  tree  the  nest  was  in  was  compact  enough  that  we  were  able  to  pull  branches  in  tight  around 
and  over  the  nest  leaving  an  opening  large  enough  for  the  laughing  thrushes  to  enter  but  not  the 
partridges.  The  partridges  did  not  interfere  with  the  laughing  thrush  breeding,  but  as  stated  earlier 
the  laughing  thrushes  did  kill  all  the  partridge  hatchlings. 


Conclusion 


White-crested  laughing  thrushes  are  a low  maintenance  species  suitable  for  breeding  on  exhibit. 
Hand-rearing  was  essential  during  the  first  years  of  breeding,  but  now  that  the  predation  problem 
has  been  solved  we  will  let  the  parents  raise  their  own  chicks  from  here  on  out  unless  unforeseen 
circumstances  deem  otherwise.  Chick  mortality  may  be  high  even  under  the  best  of  circumstances, 
but  providing  a secure  and  comfortable  habitat  and  monitoring  behavior  and  nest  activity  will  ensure 
a fair  amount  of  success  breeding  these  birds. 

Products  Mentioned  in  the  Text 

Apple  Paradise®,  manufactured  for  Marion  Zoological  by  Scenic  Bird  Food,  scenicbirdfood.com, 
2003  E Center  Circle,  Plymouth,  MN  55441,  USA. 

Ka54ee  Exact®,  manufactured  by  Kaytee  Products,  Inc.,  Chilton,  WI  53014,  USA. 

References 

Edmans,  Ian,  2008.  The  Blue-crowned  Laughing  thrush  Dryonastes  courtoisi:  Results  of  a 2007 
Husbandry  Questionnaire.  Avicultural  Magazine.  Vol.  114,  No.  4:  157-160. 

Avicultural  Society,  England. 

Mace,  M.  and  Holland,  G.  2007.  Laughing  thrushes.  Encyclopedia  of  Aviculture:  716-718. 

Glen  Holland  (ed.).  Blaine,  WA:  Hancock  House. 

Photo  Credits 

All  photos  taken  by  Lydia  Cost,  Registrar  at  the  Birmingham  Zoo. 


Types  of  Feathers 


Do  you  know  the  functions  of  each  feather?  If 
not,  look  for  the  answers  elsewhere  in  this  issue 

of  A.KF.  ( Drawings  by  Jody  Hildreth  at  www.kidwings.com) 


Bristle  Feather 


Animal  Keepers' Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5  219 


The  Management  and  Breeding  Program 

of  Roseate  Spoonbills  at  the  Palm  Beach  Zoo  at  Dreher  Park 


By  Megan  Derousie,  Avian  and  Carnivore  Keeper  III 
Palm  Beach  Zoo  at  Dreher  Park,  West  Palm  Beach,  FL 

The  Roseate  Spoonbill  {Ajaia  ajaja)  is  a large  wading  bird.  It  is  pink  in  color  with  a long, 
distinguishable  flat  bill.  This  dashing  bird  can  measure  up  to  2.5  ft.  [0.76m]  and  has  a wingspan  of 
4 ft.  [1.21m].  Adult  Roseate  spoonbills  have  bald  heads  with  a greenish  tinge  skin  color  and  a black 
band  going  across  the  back  of  the  head.  The  neck  and  upper  back  feathers  are  white  while  the  rest  of 
the  body’s  feathers  are  pink  in  color.  They  also  have  a large,  darker  pink  stripe  across  their  wings.  As 
juveniles,  the  birds  are  a paler  pink  color  and  their  heads  are  covered  in  white  feathers. 

The  range  of  the  Roseate  spoonbill  includes 
coastal  Texas,  southwestern  Louisiana,  and 
southern  Florida.  Additionally,  they  can  also 
be  found  through  the  Greater  Antilles  down 
toArgentina  and  Chile.  They  will  inhabit  both 
fresh  and  saltwater  marshes,  lakes,  swamps, 
and  rivers.  Due  to  their  habitat.  Roseate 
spoonbills  feed  on  a varied  diet  consisting 
of  small  fish,  amphibians,  invertebrates,  and 
some  plant  material.  These  wading  birds  will 
stand  in  the  shallow  waters  and  swing  their 
bills  back  and  forth  searching  for  food  items. 

Once  they  find  their  food,  they  will  grasp  it 
with  their  bill  and  swallow  it  whole. 

Roseate  spoonbills  are  typically  not  sexually  mature  until  three  years  of  age.  Their  courtship  behavior 
consists  of  bill  clapping,  head  bobbing  and  dancing,  and  the  exchange  of  nesting  material.  Once  a 
pair  is  formed,  the  female  will  build  the  nest  while  the  male  searches  and  brings  the  nesting  material 
to  her.  They  will  nest  in  mixed  colonies  with  other  species  such  as  herons,  gulls,  wood  storks,  and 
ibises.  A female  will  lay  three  to  four  pale  cream-colored  eggs,  and  both  parents  will  take  turns 
incubating  the  eggs  for  22-24  days.  The  chicks  are  bom  with  white  down  feathers  and  a very  small 
spoon-shaped  bill.  Both  parents  share  responsibilities  for  caring  and  feeding  the  chicks.  The  chicks 
will  bob  their  heads  and  vocalize  to  let  the  parents  know  they  are  hungry.  The  parents  will  then 
regurgitate  food  into  their  mouths  until  the  chicks  fledge  the  nest,  and  then  slowly  the  parents  will 
teach  them  how  to  feed  on  their  own. 

At  the  start  of  the  1900s,  a decline  in  the  Roseate  spoonbill  population  began  due  to  hunting  and 
the  demand  for  the  pink  primary  feathers  for  ladies’  fans.  As  a result,  the  Roseate  spoonbill  were 
protected  by  the  Migratory  Bird  Treaty  Act  of  1918.  However,  the  species  continued  to  decline,  and 
in  the  1930s  hit  an  all-time  low  with  only  30-40  breeding  pairs  found  in  remote  areas  of  Florida.  The 
population  in  Florida  has  now  increased  to  over  a 1000  breeding  pairs.  The  populations  in  Texas 
and  Louisiana,  however,  are  still  vulnerable  due  to  development  in  their  breeding  locations.  Roseate 
spoonbills  are  now  considered  a Species  of  Special  Concern  in  the  U.S. 

AZA-accredited  zoos  that  house  Roseate  spoonbills  take  part  in  the  Population  Management  Plan 
(PMP®)  for  this  species.  The  plan’s  purpose  is  to  manage  and  conserve  the  population  by  getting  zoos 
and  aquariums  to  voluntarily  manage  their  animals  in  accordance  with  the  recommendations  made  by 
the  PMP®  manager.  Each  institution  is  responsible  for  maintaining  a healthy,  genetically-varied,  and 
demographically  assorted  population.  With  these  standards,  it  is  important  for  each  facility  to  keep 


Wild  Ibis  and  Spoonbills  (Photo:  Keith  Lovett) 


220  Animal  Keepers'  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5 


excellent  records  on  the  birds’  breeding  and  nesting  behaviors  to  share  with  other  facilities  honing  this 
species.  This  can  ensure  a varied  population  among  zoos  and  aquariums  that  take  part  in  the  PMP®. 

The  government  is  also  involved  in  protecting  Roseate  spoonbills.  The  Coastal  Restoration  Program 
aims  to  determine  the  most  critical  human  and  natural  ecological  needs  of  the  coastal  area.  Restoring 
coastal  areas  benefits  Roseate  spoonbills  by  providing  them  with  appropriate  habitats.  The  Everglades 
Restoration  Plan  was  put  into  action  to  restore  a majority  of  the  fresh  water  supply  that  is  needed  in 
certain  areas.  The  hope  is  that  this  will  help  improve  the  environment  and  ecosystem  of  those  areas. 
With  this  in  effect,  spoonbill  breeding  locations  can  be  protected  and  the  population  will  have  the 
opportunity  to  continue  to  grow. 

In  2001  the  Palm  Beach  Zoo  at  Dreher  Park  received  2.2.2  Roseate  spoonbills  from  the  Fort  Worth 
Zoo.  When  these  birds  first  arrived,  they  were  held  off-display  in  a holding  area  for  quarantine.  In 
2002,  the  zoo  received  LI  spoonbills  from  the  Oklahoma  City  Zoo  and  an  additional  2.2  individuals 
from  the  Fort  Worth  Zoo.  While  the  2001  group  of  Roseate  spoonbills  was  still  being  housed  off- 
display,  they  were  offered  nesting  material.  There  was  not  any  nest  building  seen  or  recorded  for  this 
group  at  that  time.  After  the  2002  group  of  Roseate  spoonbills  was  introduced  into  the  first  group  and 
the  entire  group  was  placed  on  display,  they  started  showing  signs  of  nest  building  in  early  August  of 
the  same  year.  By  the  end  of  the  month,  breeding  behavior  was  observed  and  females  finally  started 
laying  eggs. 

At  the  end  of  August  2002,  the  group  had  its  first  cohort  of  chicks  hatch  out  in  one  nest.  The  nest 
had  three  hatchings  but  only  two  survived.  The  month  of  September  2002  produced  three  more 
successful  hatchings.  The  Palm  Beach  Zoo’s  group  continued  to  produce  eggs  and  successfully 
fledgled  a number  of  birds  the  following  year  in  the  months  of  February,  March,  and  June.  A majority 
of  the  juveniles  were  sent  out  to  other  zoos  for  breeding  programs,  although  some  were  kept  in  the 
group  to  maintain  our  group’s  health  and  genetic  diversity. 

The  Palm  Beach  Zoo  at  Dreher  Park  houses 
our  collection  of  Roseate  spoonbills  in 
an  exhibit  that  is  around  15  ft.  high  x 15 
ft.  wide  [4.57m  x 4.57m].  We  use  wire 
mesh  to  cover  the  exhibit.  The  birds  have 
: mayho  trees,  areca  plants,  artificial  trees, 

! and  wooden  meshed  platforms  on  which 
to  perch  and  nest.  Much  of  the  nesting 
is  done  high  up  in  the  mayho  trees  with 
' multiple  birds  nesting  around  each  tree. 

The  exhibit  also  contains  a small  waterfall 
leading  into  a stream  where  the  birds 
bathe,  drink,  feed,  and  wade.  This  stream 
I is  cleaned  out  twice  daily  due  to  the 
amount  of  feces  and  urates  all  the  birds 
produce  each  day.  We  also  use  fine  white 
sand  that  is  easy  on  the  birds’  feet,  is  easy  to  clean,  and  is  aesthetically  pleasing  for  our  visitors.  Our 
collection  of  Roseate  spoonbills  are  also  housed  with  Scarlet  Ibis  (Eudocimus  ruber)  and  Sacred  ibis 
(Threskiornis  aethiopicus).  A total  of  45  birds  are  currently  housed  together  in  the  exhibit.  The  birds 
have  never  shown  serious  aggression  in  this  exhibit.  Occasionally,  when  spoonbills  and  ibis  chicks 
fledge  the  nest,  you  will  see  some  of  the  adults  from  both  species  mount  the  chicks.  It  is  almost  as 
if  the  adults  are  showing  their  dominance  over  the  chicks.  It  is  great  having  the  species  together  for 
visitor  viewing  and  observing  the  species  displaying  natural  behaviors.  Zoo  visitors  enjoy  seeing 
both  species  to  compare  their  vibrant  colors,  and  it  is  also  a great  way  to  educate  visitors  that  in  the 
wild  all  different  species  of  water  birds  will  naturally  nest  together. 


Spoonbill  Exhibit  at  the  Palm  Beach  Zoo 

(Photo:  Megan  Derousie) 


I 


Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5  221 


The  daily  morning  staple  diet  that  is  fed  out  to  our  birds  is  Mazuri®  Brand  Flamingo  Breeder  and 
Flamingo  Powder.  This  brand  of  food  mimics  the  bird’s  wild  diet  by  providing  certain  levels  of 
vitamin  and  fish  proteins  and  contains  certain  pigment  to  help  keep  their  feathers  bright  in  color. 
As  a supplement  to  their  diet,  they  are  also  given  Nebraska®  Brand  Bird  of  Prey  meat  which  is 
scattered  around  the  exhibit  for  enrichment  or  placed  in  their  morning  diet.  In  the  evening,  they  are 
given  silverside  fish  in  multiple  pans  placed  around  the  exhibit.  They  are  given  supplements  such 
as  vitamin  B,  thiamine  E paste,  and  roxanthene  to  enhance  their  color  even  more.  Meal  worms  and 
crickets  are  also  added  for  enrichment. 

The  breeding  season  seen  at  our  zoo  will  typically  start  in  January  and  end  in  March.  After  a short 
break,  the  breeding  resumes  in  July  and  continues  until  the  end  of  September.  During  the  breeding 
season  we  offer  the  birds  a variety  of  nesting  material  daily.  We  give  them  bamboo,  vines,  ficus,  and 
varied  leafy  and  flexible  sticks  found  around  the  zoo.  The  sticks  the  birds  tend  to  prefer  are  the  ficus 
and  vines,  or  anything  that  is  leafy  and  flexible  enough  to  be  manipulated  into  the  nest.  The  birds 
exhibit  behaviors  seen  in  the  wild  as  the  female  will  stand  and  wait  to  build  the  nest  while  the  male 
searches  and  brings  her  the  material.  This  is  why  it  is  important  to  provide  them  daily  with  varied 
sticks  since  the  female  will  occasionally  drop  the  sticks  the  male  gives  her  when  she  is  unsatisfied 
with  the  selection.  They  will  also  steal  sticks  from  other  nests  and  destroy  their  own  nest  if  they  are 
not  satisfied  with  the  nesting  material. 

Right  before  the  breeding  season  you  will  see  a lot  of  head  bobbing,  dancing,  bill  sparring,  and  the 
exchanging  of  sticks  between  paired  individuals.  The  male  will  do  most  of  this  behavior  to  impress 
the  female.  Observers  will  also  hear  a lot  of  vocalization  within  the  group.  The  adult  male  and  female 
in  a breeding  pair  will  fly  back  and  forth  to  the  nest  and  will  be  seen  copulating  in  the  nest  once  it  is 
completed.  Once  the  first  egg  is  laid,  this  behavior  does  stop.  The  same  pair  continues  this  courtship 
behavior  throughout  the  year  and  will  usually  pair  again  the  following  year  and  repeat  the  cycle. 

The  Palm  Beach  Zoo  at  Dreher  Park’s  breeding  program  consists  of  three  to  four  pairs  each  year. 
They  will  usually  clutch  out  two  times  a year.  If  they  start  early  in  the  season,  or  their  first  clutch  is 
unsuccessful,  they  will  have  three  clutches.  They  will  lay  three  to  four  eggs  per  nest.  At  times  they 
will  push  an  egg  out,  or  an  egg  will  accidentally  fall  out  of  the  nest.  In  that  case,  they  will  then  lay 
another  egg  before  incubating  the  clutch. 

We  are  very  hands-off  when  it  comes  to  incubating  an  egg.  The  parents  are  very  good  incubators, 
which  means  we  have  never  had  to  resort  to  artificial  incubation.  The  female  will  most  often  be  seen 
incubating  the  eggs.  She  will  get  off  the  nest  very  briefly  to  eat,  and  then  the  male  will  take  over  the 
duty  of  incubating  the  egg.  If  either  parent  sees  any  disturbance  around  the  nest,  both  parents  fly  over 
to  sit  and  protect  the  eggs. 

Both  parents  are  excellent  at  guarding  the  chicks  from  other  birds  and  keepers.  The  birds  tend  to  nest 
very  close  to  each  other,  so  you  will  see  the  parents  doing  a great  deal  of  bill  sparring  with  each  other 
when  one  steps  over  the  line  and  gets  too  close.  When  the  keepers  are  checking  nests,  a portable 
security  mirror  will  be  used,  and  the  parents  immediately  fly  over  to  the  nest  and  cover  the  chicks  and 
spar  with  the  mirror.  This  can  make  it  hard  to  monitor  the  chicks,  so  keepers  will  stand  and  listen  to 
make  sure  they  are  still  hearing  the  chicks  vocalize,  showing  us  they  are  still  strong.  Once  the  chicks 
are  fully  standing  in  the  nest,  it  is  a lot  easier  to  monitor  them  from  a distance. 

The  Roseate  spoonbills  are  very  good  at  caring  for  their  young.  When  the  chicks  first  hatch  they  have 
a very  quiet  feeding  vocalization.  Once  the  chicks  are  strong  enough,  they  will  start  standing,  head 
bobbing,  and  will  break  into  a loud  burst  of  feeding  vocalization  toward  the  parents  every  time  they  are 
hungry  or  the  parents  fly  over  to  the  nest.  After  about  a month,  the  chicks  will  begin  exploring  around 
the  nest  while  the  parents  continue  to  feed  them.  At  around  a month  and  a half  of  age,  the  chicks  are 
flying  back  in  forth  from  one  nest  to  another,  while  staying  very  close  to  the  original  nest  since  the 
parents  are  still  feeding  them.  After  two  months,  the  chicks  will  be  down  on  the  ground  exploring,  but 


222  Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5 


I 

I 


spoonbill  parents  protecting  chicks.  {Photo:  Keith  Lovett) 


The  zoo’s  role  in  protecting  the 
eggs  and  offspring  is  relatively 
hands-off.  Since  the  parents  do  a 
great  job,  zoo  staff  prefer  not  to  interfere  too  much.  The  one  major  problem  we  have  encountered  is 
the  eggs  or  chicks  falling  out  of  the  nest.  The  zoo  has  developed  a very  successful  solution  by  placing 
a mesh-type  material  under  all  the  nests.  During  nesting  season  the  keepers  will  check  the  nests  first 
thing  in  the  morning  to  see  if  anything  has  fallen  out.  If  an  egg  or  chick  is  found,  it  will  be  placed 
back  into  the  nest.  Fortunately  the  parents  have  always  accepted  the  egg  or  chick  back  into  the  nest 
without  any  issues.  The  mesh  cushions  the  egg  from  cracking  when  it  falls  and  saves  the  chick  from 
drowning  in  the  stream  or  being  injured  by  the  fall.  During  the  summer  months,  an  increased  issue 
with  chicks  who  are  still  in  the  nests  are  signs  of  heat  stress.  To  remedy  this,  a large  tarp  is  placed  onto 
the  top  of  the  exhibit  to  give  the  chicks  and  nests  more  shade.  By  closely  monitoring  the  behaviors  of 
the  chicks  and  our  surrounding  environment,  we  have  been  able  to  solve  these  issues. 


will  still  return  to  the  nest  at  night. 
The  chicks  will  continue  to  beg 
for  food,  and  although  the  parents 
will  begin  to  ignore  them  more 
and  more,  they  will  still  give  them 
an  occasional  feeding.  The  more 
the  parents  ignore  the  chicks,  the 
sooner  the  chicks  catch  on  and 
attempt  to  eat  on  their  own  at  the 
group  feeders.  In  the  beginning, 
the  chicks  are  very  hesitant  due  to 
the  occasional  shoving  from  the 
other  birds,  but  once  the  chicks 
know  the  pecking  order,  they 
become  more  independent  when 
eating  with  the  group. 


Spoonbill  chicks  in  nest.  (Photo:  Keith  Lovett) 


Zoo  staff  continue  to  learn  about 
the  Roseate  spoonbills’  breeding 
behavior  with  daily  observations, 
which  has  led  us  to  have  a very 
successful  breeding  program 
over  the  past  couple  of  years.  The 
zoo  has  had  spoonbills  on  exhibit 
for  close  to  eight  years  and  every 
year  they  have  successfully 
raised  offspring.  In  addition,  the 
number  of  chicks  has  increased 
each  year.  Two-thousand-nine 
was  our  most  successful  year 
with  eight  chicks  successfully 
fledging  the  nest.  With  the 
knowledge  we  have  gained,  the 
spoonbills’  breeding  success 
rate  will  hopefully  continue  to 
increase. 


With  our  proven  and  continued  success  we  are  able  to  send  out  healthy,  genetically- varied  breeding 
j pairs  or  singletons  to  other  zoos  and  aquariums  around  the  country.  Varying  the  breeding  line  can 
i help  to  ensure  a healthy  captive  population  both  at  our  zoo  and  at  other  institutions  that  house  this 


Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5  223 


vibrant  and  unique  wading  bird.  With  a continued  growth  in  the  Roseate  spoonbill  breeding  program 
in  zoos  and  aquariums,  and  assistance  with  the  protection  of  wild  population  breeding  locations  from 
the  government,  there  is  hope  that  the  species  will  continue  to  make  a comeback. 

The  Palm  Beach  Zoo  at  Dreher  Park  knows  the  importance  of  educating  visitors  about  these  beautiful 
birds  not  only  in  the  wild,  but  also  in  captivity.  It  is  our  mission  to  promote  field  projects,  educate 
the  public,  and  provide  funding  for  research  and  reintroductions  back  into  the  wild  if  and  when 
appropriate. 

References 

“Association  of  Zoos  and  Aquariums.”  Animal  Management.  1997-2009.  Association  of 
Zoos  and  Aquariums.  January  2010.  <www.aza.org>. 

Ehrlich,  P.R.,  Dobkin,  D.S.  and  D.  Wheye.  1988.  The  Birder’s  Handbook. 

Simon  and  Schuster,  New  York. 

Farrand,  J.  Jr.  ed.  1983.  The  Audubon  Society  Master  Guide  to  Birding  Vol.  1. 

Alfred  A.  Knopf,  Inc.  New  York. 

Netherton,  J.  1994.  At  the  Water’s  Edge:  Wading  Birds  of  North  America. 

Voyageur  Press,  Stillwater,  MN. 

“Palm  Beach  Conservation.”  Species  Survival  Plan®.  2009.  Palm  Beach  Zoo. 

January  2010.  <www.palmbeachzoo.org>. 

“The  Journey  To  Restore  America’s  Everglades.”  About  CERP:  Brief  Overview. 

December  2000.  The  Comprehensive  Everglades  Restoration  Plan  (CERP). 

January  2010.  <www.evergladesplan.org>. 

“WaterMarks.”  Louisiana  Coastal  Wetlands  Planning,  Protection  and  Restoration  News. 

September  2009.  WaterMarks.  January  2010.  <www.lacoast.gov>. 


Types  of  Feathers Did  You  Know? 

Feathers  come  in  many  different  shapes,  but  all  of  them  can  be  classified  into  six  different  types  of 
feathers.  The  feathers  we  are  used  to  seeing  are  flight  and  contour.  Down  feathers  are  what  we  use 
in  pillows,  quilts,  and  jackets.  The  other  three  feathers,  semiplume,  filoplume,  and  bristle,  are  not  as 
common,  but  still  useful  to  birds. 

1 . Flight  feathers  are  found  two  places  on  birds:  the  wings  and  tail.  Flight  feathers  are  long,  and  on 
the  wings,  have  one  side  of  the  vane  wider  than  the  other.  They  also  have  stronger  barbules  which 
give  them  more  strength  for  flight. 

2.  Contour  feathers  give  shape  and  color  to  the  bird.  They  are  found  everywhere  except  the  beak, 
legs,  and  feet.  Contour  feathers  are  colored  only  at  the  ends  (the  only  part  that  we  see).  At  its  base, 
a contour  feather  becomes  downy  which  helps  insulate  the  bird. 

3.  Down  feathers  have  little  or  no  shaft.  They  are  soft  and  fluffy.  Down  feathers  help  insulate  birds 
by  trapping  air.  Some  birds,  such  as  herons,  have  special  down  feathers  called  powder  down  which 
breaks  up  into  a fine  powder.  The  bird  then  spreads  this  fine  powder  all  over  its  body  to  act  as  a water 
repellant. 

4.  Semiplume  feathers  are  a cross  between  down  and  contour  feathers.  Unlike  down,  they  do  have 
a well  formed  shaft.  However,  they  do  not  have  well  developed  barbicels  which  make  them  soft. 
Semiplume  feathers  are  found  underneath  contour  feathers  and  are  used  for  insulation. 

5.  Bristle  feathers  are  very  stiff  with  only  a few  barbs  found  at  the  base.  Bristle  feathers  are  found 
around  the  mouth  of  insect  eating  birds  where  they  act  as  a funnel.  They  can  also  be  found  around 
the  eyes  where  they  work  like  eyelashes. 

6.  Filoplume  feathers  are  incredibly  small.  They  have  a tuft  of  barbs  at  the  end  of  the  shaft.  Unlike 
other  feathers  which  are  attached  to  muscle  for  movement,  filoplume  feathers  are  attached  to  nerve 
endings.  These  feathers  send  messages  to  the  brain  that  give  infonnation  about  the  placement  of 

feathers  for  flight,  insulation,  and  preening.  (Written  by  Jody  HUdreth  at  www.kidwings.com) 


224  Animal  Keepers’ Eorum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5 


Puppets:  Construction  and  Use  for  the  Hand-rearing  of  Birds 

By  Giane  Giujfre,  Assistant  Keeper;  Bob  Thornton,  Assistant  Lead  Keeper; 
and  Colleen  Lynch,  Curator 
Lincoln  Park  Zoo,  Chicago,  IL 


Introduction 

Successftilly  rearing  chicks  is  always  a challenge.  While  parent-rearing  is  the  desired  method  of 
! chick  rearing  in  most  instances,  it  is  sometimes  necessary  to  artificially  rear  birds  due  to  a variety  of 
factors:  death  of  an  incubating  or  brooding  parent,  egg  destruction,  poor  parental  care  of  chicks,  risk 
of  disease  transmission  from  parents  to  chicks,  threat  from  aggressive  exhibit  mates,  competition 
or  siblicide  from  clutch  mates,  etc.  If  a species  is  in  peril,  artificially  rearing  chicks  is  often  the 
recommended  method  to  increase  the  number  of  clutches  produced  per  season  as  well  as  to  ensure 
chick  survival  against  any  of  the  risks  listed  above. 

Artificial  rearing  can  be  done  in  several  ways.  Puppet-rearing  may  be  the  most  effective  artificial 
method  in  use  to  avoid  inappropriate  imprinting  of  chicks  on  their  keepers.  With  careful  elimination 
of  any  chick-human  interaction  and  appropriate  visual  cues  of  an  adult  conspecific,  a puppet 
provides  some  of  the  basic  stimuli  a juvenile  bird  focuses  on  when  housed  with  and  cared  for  by 
live  conspecifics.  Though  species-specific  results  vary  and  parent-rearing  is  most  often  the  preferred 
method,  puppet-rearing  gives  us  an  alternative  that  may  be  beneficial  to  a bird’s  ability  to  live  socially 
with  conspecifics  and  achieve  reproductive  success. 

The  natural  history  of  the  species  in  your  care,  their  degree  of  sociality  and  a similar  comparison 
between  parent-reared  and  artificially-reared  birds  may  provide  guidelines  necessary  for  choosing 
the  method  to  artificially  rear  birds  (Wallace,  1994).  Not  all  situations  allow  for  careful  comparisons 
between  parent-  and  puppet-reared  chicks,  and  in  many  cases  the  benefit  is  assumed  based  on  studies 
like  the  ones  referenced  below.  Puppet-rearing  may  be  most  useful  in  species  having  a smaller  clutch 
size,  thatnaturally  leads  to  more  parental  dedication  to  individual  young,  and  a higher  degree  of 
sociality.  Puppet-rearing  may  be  considered  of  great  importance  especially  in  cases  where  individuals 
are  reared  for  reintroduction  programs.  In  these  instances,  rearing  method  may  not  only  influence 
future  reproductive  success  but  also  the  ability  of  the  released  individual  to  survive  in  the  wild 
j (Wallace,  1994).  The  Andean  {Vultur  gryphus)  and  California  condors  (Gymnogyps  californianus) 
illustrated  little  behavioral  difference  or  change  in  success  rate  between  released  condors  that  were 
parent-reared  or  puppet-reared.  Bald  eagles  (Haliaeetus  leucocephalus),  a more  social  species, 
developed  pair  bonds  typical  of  the  species  later  in  life  when  raised  with  puppets  as  eaglets  (Simons 
et.  al.,  1988).  Hawaiian  crows  (Corvus  tropicus)  were  taught  to  socialize  with  a puppet  crow  and  to 
forage  for  a variety  of  diet  items  (Halliday  and  Slater,  1994).  For  Operation  Migration,  a non-profit 
organization  dedicated  to  saving  a species  in  peril,  a full-body  costume  is  used  for  Whooping  crane 
{Grus  americana)  rearing,  socialization,  and  teaching  chicks  to  forage,  and  an  ultralight  aircraft  leads 
them  on  their  natural  migration  fwww.operationmigration.org). 

The  Imprinting  Process 

' It  has  been  over  a century  since  amateur  biologist  Douglas  Spalding  described  parental  recognition  in 
chickens  and  over  70  years  since  Konrad  Lorenz  (1935)  first  used  the  term  imprinting  to  describe  the 
means  by  which  species  recognition  is  learned.  Since  that  time,  a large  body  of  research  has  emerged 
, in  support  of  Lorenz’s  theories  on  filial  and  sexual  imprinting  (Sluckin,  1972;  Hess,  1973).  Much  of 
I this  research  has  influenced  methods  of  rearing  animals  in  captivity,  especially  birds. 

I 

I Imprinting  occurs  when  an  innate  pattern  of  behavior  becomes  associated  with  a specific  object 
1 through  learning  during  a “sensitive  period”  (Lorenz,  1935).  See  Figure  1.  Imprinting  behaviors  are 
I typically  differentiated  into  filial  (parental,  sibling)  and  sexual  (mate)  imprinting. 

Animal  Keepers'  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5  225 


Filial  imprinting  occurs  when  a chick  restricts  its  filial  attachment  and  following  response  to  a 
parent,  sibling,  or  other  stimulus.  This  oceurs 
relatively  early  in  development,  even  in  the 
first  12  hours  for  some  precocial  species 
(Bateson,  1988a).  Behavior  such  as  this  has 
been  deseribed  in  a taxonomically  diverse 
eolleetion  of  avian  species.  Though  there  is 
a “sensitive  period”  in  whieh  birds  will  prefer 
companionship  with  whatever  object  they 
have  imprinted  on  in  the  first  40  days  of  life, 
more  current  researeh  has  shown  that  this 
faetor  is  not  stable  unless  “validated”  by  an 
experience  later  in  life,  i.e.  mate  choice.  If 
exposure  to  conspecifics  is  extended,  i.e.  to  50 
days,  the  sensitive  period  is  invalid  perhaps 
due  to  extended  exposure  to  siblings  and  the 
adverse  reaction  to  inbreeding  (Hogan  et  al., 

1997).  Often  associated  with  this  recognition 
of  conspecifics  is  the  development  of  a fear 
response  to  novel  objeets  such  as  non 
conspecifics  (Bateson,  1988b). 

Additional  early  social  experiences  have  been  demonstrated  to  influence  subsequent  sexual  behavior 
and  mate  choiee  (Immelmann,  1972).  This  is  referred  to  as  sexual  imprinting.  It  is  known  to  occur 
later  in  development  than  filial  imprinting,  with  a sensitive  period  until  fledging  in  some  altricial 
species  (Bateson,  1988b).  While  siblings  may  provide  a model  for  filial  imprinting,  the  model  for 
sexual  imprinting  appears  more  often  to  be  the  source  of  food,  usually  the  parent. 

Proper  filial  and  sexual  imprinting  are  critical  to  the  captive  propagation  of  many  avian  species,  both 
to  maintain  sustainable  captive  populations  and  to  provide  suitable  individuals  for  reintroduction 
or  other  conservation  programs.  In  the  best  of  both  worlds,  all  captive  animals  would  be  parent- 
reared  and  inappropriate  imprinting  would  not  be  an  issue,  but  this  is  not  always  possible.  While 
cross-fostering  to  other  speeies  may  be  an  effective  method  of  avoiding  labor-intensive  hand-rearing, 
imprinting  problems  may  result  (Immelman,  1969). 

Artifieial  rearing  methods  can  be  divided  into  the  major  categories  below: 

1)  Hand-rearing:  raising  a chick  without  taking  any  precaution  to  hide  the  human  form. 

2)  Ghost-rearing:  raising  a chick  while  hiding  the  human  form  and  voice.  This  involves  keeping 
way  glass  or  Mylar®,  ghosts  (a  sheet  over  the  keeper’s  head  and  body),  mirrors  and 
minimizing  light. 

3)  Puppet-rearing:  raising  a chick  while  hiding  the  human  form  and  voice  while  using  a puppet 
form  of  the  adult  plumage  coloration  intending  to  create  species  recognition  in  the  chick 
during  a critical  stage  of  imprinting. 

In  reality,  these  methods  are  not  mutually  exclusive  and  the  artificial  rearing  of  most  chicks  represents 
some  point  on  a eontinuum  that  combines  aspects  of  all  these  methods. 

Birds  that  imprint,  either  filially  or  sexually,  on  an  inappropriate  object  are  often  referred  to 
as  “malimprinted.”  Including  puppets  in  the  rearing  process  is  the  most  aggressive  measure  that 
can  be  taken  to  avoid  malimprinting.  While  puppet-rearing  can  be  more  labor-intensive  and  time 
consuming  than  more  traditional  rearing  methods,  this  technique  has  been  used  with  great  success  in 
a number  of  avian  speeies.  While  a chick  can  imprint  on  objects  that  are  dissimilar  to  their  parents, 
they  will  imprint  more  strongly  and  rapidly  on  objects  which  closely  approximate  their  parents. 
Species-specific  preferences  exist  for  certain  colors  and  forms.  Therefore  puppets  need  not  be  perfect 


Figure.  1 Demonstrates  the  level  of  imprinting 
sensitivity  based  on  different  stages  of  development 

in  birds  (Reproduced from  Fox,  1995), 


226  Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5 


representations  or  made  to  absolute  scale,  but  must  present  basic  cues,  and  suitable  puppets  are  easily 
constructed  for  many  species. 

Puppets  and  Kingfishers  at  Lincoln  Park  Zoo 

The  Guam  Micronesian  Kingfisher  {Todiramphus  cinnamominus  cinnamominus)  is  extinct  in  the 
wild.  Lincoln  Park  Zoo  has  made  collaborative  conservation  efforts  with  zoos  across  the  country, 
and  is  an  active  player  in  the  Micronesian  Kingfisher  Species  Survival  Plan®.  Lincoln  Park  Zoo 
produces  several  kingfisher  chicks  on  average  annually,  and  has  supplemented  this  small  population 
; (currently  just  over  100  individuals)  with  41  chicks  total  since  1991  (Taxon  Report,  8 Dec.  2009). 

According  to  the  Micronesian  Kingfisher  SSP®  Husbandry  Manual,  kingfisher  chicks  are 
recommended  to  be  hand-reared  birds  due  to  the  risk  of  inadequate  parental  care  from  captive  adult 
birds  and  the  inconsistency  seen  even  with  experienced  pairs  (Bahner  and  Diebold,  1998).  Since 
1 Micronesian  kingfishers  normally  clutch  two  eggs,  chick  survival  rates  in  the  nest  could  also  decline 
due  to  siblicide  or  resource  allocation.  Parental  rearing  of  a single  chick  offers  beneficial  experience 
for  adult  birds  and  the  single  parent-reared  chick  maximizes  its  benefits.  The  recommendation  to 
I artificially  incubate  and  hand-rear  the  second  fertile  egg  in  a clutch  allows  for  increased  representation 
of  individuals  in  a population  and  maximum  population  growth  (Bahner  pers.  comm.,  2009). 

For  several  reasons  Micronesian  kingfishers  were  the  chosen  species  for  which  to  build  a puppet 
and  puppet-rear  program  at  Lincoln  Park  Zoo  We  currently  house  two  successful  breeding  pairs 
and  as  mentioned,  this  species  has  SSP®  recommendations  to  hand-rear.  As  a species,  Micronesian 
kingfishers  are  relatively  hardy,  easy  to  rear  chicks.  While  the  malimprinting  of  chicks  has  not  been  a 
noted  problem  in  the  SSP®,  puppets  are  regularly  used  by  some  other  SSP®-participating  institutions. 
This  species  may  not  easily  imprint  on  keepers,  but  the  use  of  puppets  may  benefit  the  chicks  in  some 
ways  yet  to  be  determined:  if  exposed  to  the  visual  cues  of  their  adult  conspecific  from  the  beginning, 
the  benefits  of  their  familiarity  could  be  seen  in  introductions  and  future  breeding  successes  both  in 
captive  situations  and  potential  releases  to  the  wild. 

This  being  said,  puppet-rearing  a species  that  is  not  a high  risk  for  imprinting  offers  a beneficial 
experience  for  staff  learning  the  puppet-rearing  technique  for  the  first  time.  Working  with  a species 
that  is  unlikely  to  imprint  on  humans  is  a good  opportunity  to  develop  and  perfect  the  skill  of  puppet- 
rearing for  future,  more  challenging  species. 

Constructing  a Puppet 

Materials: 

A bird  to  serve  as  a model  Non-toxic  acrylic  paints 

Bondo®  Auto  Repair  Compound  * Small  putty  knife 

Dust  masks/goggles/other  personal  protection  equipment  Accessories  such  as  eyes 
I Styrofoam®  forms  Small  carving  knife 

Headless  bolt  and  two  nuts  Rotary  tool 

Toothpicks  Carving  and  sanding  bits 

Gorilla  Glue®  Velcro®  strips 

* Bondo®  is  used  as  the  construction  material  as  it  is  durable  and  easily  disinfected. 

Procedure: 

Step  1 - Creating  a mold 

I Using  Styrofoam®  forms  that  approximate  the  size  and  shape  of  the  finished  puppet,  begin  to  cut  out 
I the  shape  of  the  bird’s  head.  Balls  can  be  used  to  form  heads,  cones  for  beaks,  etc.  {Figure  2). 

If  more  than  one  piece  of  Styrofoam®  is  required  to  complete  the  head,  bridge  them  together  with 
I toothpicks.  The  finished  mold  should  be  slightly  smaller  than  the  desired  finished  puppet  {Figure  i). 

I To  form  an  articulate  mandible,  a second,  independent  form  will  be  necessary. 


Animal  Keepers'  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5  111 


Step  2 - Coatififi  the  mold 
Mix  a small  portion  of  the  Bondo"  with  the 
activator  as  directed  b\'  the  maiiLiraeturcr.  With 
a putty  knife,  begin  to  coat  tlie  St\  rofoam’'  mold 
with  the  Bondo'f  Remember  to  lea\e  a large 


Figure  2.  Styrofoam  in  many  shapes  and  sizes 
can  be  used.  ll’hoit)  h\  (lidiui  (Imllu'i 

enough  opening  in  the  bottom-rear  of  the  bottom 
of  the  puppet  large  enough  to  allow  its  operation. 
Continue  to  add  Bondo'^,  approximating  the  bird’s 
Figure  3.  Styrofoam  should  be  shaped  to  features,  until  it  is  more  than  % inch  thick  (the 
approximate  desired  size  of  puppet.  (Photo  by  Giana  thicker  it  is  the  more  durable  your  puppet  will  be) 

{Figured).  Adjusting  the  amount  of  activator  you 
add  will  allow  you  to  influence  the  moldability 
and  drying  time  of  the  Bondo®  to  help  create  features,  which  may  be  added  in  several  layers.  At  this 
point  you  need  only  create  abstract  features,  not  fine  details.  For  long  thin  beaks,  leave  the  underside 
uncovered  and  the  Styrofoam®  exposed. 


Figure  4.  Coat  the  mold  with  multiple  layers  of 

Bondo®.  (Photo  by  Giana  Giujfre) 


Step  3 - Removing  the  mold 

Once  the  Bondo®  is  in  the  desired  shape  and 
completely  dry  the  mold  can  be  removed  to 
hollow  out  the  puppet.  It  can  be  scraped  out  with 
a spoon  or  knife.  Back-fill  the  beak  to  create  a 
solid  piece  of  Bondo®.  Smooth  out  the  inside 
of  the  puppet  with  the  sanding  bit  on  the  rotary 
tool.  The  smoother  it  is  the  more  comfortable  it 
will  be  to  use. 

Step  4 - Shaping  the  puppet 

Using  the  carving  and  sanding  bits  of  the  rotary 
tool,  smooth,  shape  and  detail  the  puppet’s 
exterior.  Use  a dust  mask  and  work  outside.  At 
this  point  the  puppet  can  become  as  detailed  and 
accurate  as  your  artistic  abilities  allow  {Figure  5). 


Step  5 - Painting  the  puppet 

Make  sure  all  of  the  dust  is  removed  and  use  a non- 
toxic acrylic  paint.  A water-based  topcoat  can  be 
used  to  protect  the  paint  and  accessories  such  as 
eyes  and  other  features  can  be  added  at  this  time 
{Figure  6). 


Figure  5.  The  Bondo®  is  smoothed  and  the  final 
shape  is  created  using  a rotary  tool.  (Photo  by  Giana 

Giujfre) 


228  Animal  Keepers'  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5 


step  6 - Articulating  the  mandible  of  larger  puppets 

Determine  the  point  at  which  the  mandible  will 
pivot  and  mark  this  spot  on  both  sides  of  the  head 
and  mandible.  Use  the  rotary  tool  with  a drill  to 
drill  a straight  hole  through  all  of  these  points.  Pass 
the  headless  bolt  through  these  points  to  articulate, 
placing  a nut  on  either  side  of  the  mandible  to  hold 
it  centered  on  the  bolt.  Back-fill  the  holes  on  either 
1 side  of  the  head  with  Bondo®  to  hold  the  bolt  in 
I place  and  touch  up  with  paint.  For  smaller  puppets 
without  an  articulated  mandible,  cut  a hole  through 
which  feeding  forceps  can  be  inserted  {Figure  7). 

Step  7 - Create  a sleeve 

Create  a sleeve  to  cover  the  forearm  of  the  puppeteer. 
This  can  be  as  simple  as  the  cutoff  sleeve  of  a 
sweatshirt  resembling  the  plumage  color  of  the  adult 
bird  {Figures  8 and  9). 


Figure  6.  The  puppet  is  painted. 

(Photo  by  Giana  Giujfre) 


\ Figure  7.  The  puppet  mandible  can  be 
articulated  or  forceps  inserted  through  a 

hole.  (Photo  by  Giana  Giujfre) 


Figure  8.  The  final  puppet. 

(Photo  by  Giana  Giujfre) 


I 

i 


Figure  9.  The  puppet  approximates  the 

COnspecific.  (Photo  by  Colleen  Lynch) 


i 

1 


Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5  229 


Ten  tips  for  successful  puppet-rearing 

1)  Never  let  the  chick  see  you.  Whether  you  are  feeding,  weighing,  or  cleaning  it  is  important  that  i 
the  chick  not  see  humans. 

2)  Wear  a ghost.  A camouflage  cover  over  the  head,  arms,  and  body  breaks  up  the  human  form  and 
focuses  attention  on  the  puppet. 

3)  Use  a feeding  blind.  Simple  visual  barriers  containing  one-way  glass  or  Mylar®  can  be  easy  to 
work  with  and  eliminate  the  feeder  from  sight.  Remember,  many  one-way  barriers  are  only  truly  one- 
way when  it  is  light  on  the  chick’s  side  and  dark  on  the  feeder’s  side. 

4)  Practice  using  your  puppet,  if  possible,  before  the  chick’s  eyes  open.  This  will  make  you  a pro 
before  you  need  to  be. 

5)  Begin  using  the  puppet  during  filial  imprinting,  before  a fear  of  novel  items  sets  in. 

6)  Puppet-feed  even  if  chicks  are  being  raised  in  a clutch  or  with  mirrors  to  provide  for  filial  and 
sexual  imprinting. 

7)  Allow  the  chick  to  spend  “quality  time”  with  the  puppet  before  and  after  feedings. 

8)  Do  not  talk  in  the  chick-rearing  area  and  minimize  other  human  noises  as  much  as  possible, 
especially  at  feeding  time. 

9)  Play  audio  tapes  of  the  vocalizations  of  the  adult  birds  of  the  appropriate  species.  This  will 
stimulate  the  chick  to  feed  and  encourage  auditory  imprinting. 

10)  Be  consistent.  Be  patient.  Puppets  can  be  difficult  to  work  with  until  one  becomes  accustomed 
to  them,  but  the  end  result  is  often  well  worth  the  effort 

Products  Mentioned  in  the  Text 

3M  Bondo®  Body  Filler  Original  Formula 

3700  Atlanta  Industrial  Pkwy.  NW,  Atlanta,  GA  30331  www.Bondo.com 

References 

Bahner  B,  Baltz  A,  Diebold  E,  editors.  1998.  Micronesian  Kingfisher 
Species  Survival  Plan  Husbandry  Manual:  First  Edition. 

Bateson  P.  1988a.  Preferences  for  Close  Relations  in  Japanese  Quail.  Ouellet  H,  editor. 

Ottawa:  University  of  Ottawa  Press.  Acta  XIX  Congressus  Intemationalis 
Omithologici  (1):  961-972. 

Bateson  P.  1988b.  The  Sensitive  Periods  for  Imprinting  in  Birds.  Ouellet  H,  editor. 

Ottawa:  University  of  Ottawa  Press.  Acta  XIX  Congressus  Intemationalis 
Omithologici  (1):  1116-1126. 

Fox  N.  1995.  Understanding  the  Bird  of  Prey.  Blaine:  Hancock  House.  ’ 

Gage  LJ,  DVM,  Duerr  RS,  DVM.  2007.  Hand-Rearing  Birds.  Ames:  Blackwell  Publishing. 

Halliday  T,  Slater  PJB.  1994.  Behaviour  and  Evolution.  New  York:  Cambridge  University  Press. 

Hogan  JA,  Bolhuis  JJ,  Kmijt  JP.  1997.  Causal  Mechanisms  of  Behavioural  Development. 

New  York:  Cambridge  University  Press. 

Hess  EH.  1973.  Imprinting:  Early  Experience  and  the  Development  Psychobiology  of  Attachment. 

Vancouver:  Van  Nostrand  Company. 

Immelmann  K.  1972.  Sexual  and  Other  Long-term  Aspects  of  Imprinting  in  Birds 
and  other  Species.  Advances  in  the  Study  of  Behavior.  (4):  147- 174. 

Immelmann  K.  1969.  Song  Development  in  the  Zebra  Finch  and  Other  Estrelid  Finches. 

Bird  Vocalizations.  Cambridge  University  Press. 

Lorenz  K.  1935.  The  Companion  in  the  Environment  of  the  Bird.  Berlin:  Deutschen 
Omithologicschen  Gesellschaft. 

“Our  Whooping  Crane  Story.”  10  Dec.  2009.  Operation  Migration:  Our  Work.  December  2009. 

<http://www.operationmigration.org/work_wcranes.html>. 

Simons  T,  Sherrod  S,  Collopy  M,  Jenkins  M.  1988.  Restoring  the  Bald  Eagle. 

American  Scientist.  (76):  253-260. 

Sluckin  W.  1972.  Imprinting  and  Early  Learning.  London:  Methuen. 

Taxon  Report  for  Halcyon  Cinnamomina.  Lincoln  Park  Zoo.  8 Dec.  2009. 

Wallace  MR  1994.  Control  of  Behavioral  Development  in  the  Context  of 

Reintroduction  Programs  for  Birds.  Los  Angeles:  Wiley-Liss,  Inc. 

Zoo  Biology.  (13):  491-499. 


230  Animal  Keepers'  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5 


Raising  Red- vented  Bulbuls  (Pycnonotus  cafer) 
In  Tropic  World  Asia  Bird  Holding 


By  Laurie  Morgan,  Senior  Keeper 
Bird/Herp  Department 
Brookfield  Zoo,  Brookfield,  IL 

On  3 December  2008  we  acquired  three  pairs  and  a single  female  of  Red- vented  Bulbuls  (Pycnonotus 
cafer)  from  Disney’s  Animal  Kingdom  and  are  currently  participating  in  a population  management 
study  with  this  species.  Prior  to  this  acquisition  the  staff  at  the  Brookfield  Zoo  had  very  limited 
experience  with  the  captive  management  of  bulbuls.  Once  released  from  quarantine  on  6 January 
2009  these  birds  were  moved  to  Tropic  World-Asia  bird  holding.  Our  goal  for  this  species  is  to 
encourage  reproduction  to  help  sustain  their  captive  population,  and  ultimately  create  a non-breeding 
flock  to  release  to  the  larger  exhibit.  This  flock  will  serve  as  a genetic  reserve. 


Skylights  over  the  exhibit  provide  natural  lighting 
throughout  the  year.  There  are  1000-watt  metal  Halide 
lamps  over  the  public  area  that  are  set  to  be  on  from  0500 
to  0930hrs  and  again  from  1430  to  ISOOhrs.  These  lights 
assist  with  exhibit  plant  growth  and  provide  supplemental 
lighting  on  overcast  days.  While  these  lights  and  the 
skylights  are  not  directly  above  the  bird  holding  area, 
they  do  provide  general  lighting. 

The  holding  area  is  divided  into  six  aviaries  with  a 
common  meshed  in  service  area  (a  hallway  that  has 
access  all  the  aviaries).  This  design  provides  a safety  area 
in  case  a bird  was  to  escape  from  an  aviary.  The  aviaries 
are  8 ft.  high  x 4 ft.  wide  x 7 ft.  long  [2.43m  x 1.22m  x 
2.13m].  Each  aviary  has  solid  sight  barriers  on  the  sides 
between  the  aviaries.  The  ceiling  and  the  ends  of  the 
aviaries  are  mesh  (facing  the  exhibit  and  service  area). 
All  of  the  aviaries  are  equipped  with  a variety  of  natural 
branch  perching.  Three  of  the  aviaries  housed  pairs  of 
bulbuls  during  the  2009  breeding  season. 


Tropic  World-  Asia  Bird  Holding 

Tropic  World  is  a large  exhibit  as  long  as  a street  block  and  is  divided  into  three  areas  (South  America, 
Asia,  and  Africa).  The  Asia  exhibit  of  Tropic  World  is  75  ft.  high  x 1 10  ft.  wide  x 120  ft.  long  [22.86m 
X 33.52m  x 36.57m].  Tropic  World-Asia  bird  holding  is  located  above  the  general  public  viewing  area 
at  one  end  of  the  exhibit.  Sound  echoes  throughout  the  exhibit  and  on  a crowded  day  the  noise  can  be 
extraordinarily  loud.  The  exhibit  is  temperature  controlled,  with  the  temperatures  being  maintained 
throughout  the  year  in  the  75°F  [~23.9°C]  range. 


Tropic  World-Asia  view  of  the  bird 
holding  from  the  guest  walk. 

(Photo  by  Jim  Schulz) 


Although  the  aviaries  are  small,  having  visual  access  to  the  huge  exhibit  may  give  the  birds  a bigger 
sense  of  their  environment.  Since  the  aviaries  are  at  one  end  of  the  exhibit,  calls  from  the  birds 
bounce  off  the  opposite  wall  of  the  exhibit  creating  echoes. 

I 

I Not  having  previous  experience  with  bulbuls,  the  set-up  for  each  aviary  was  slightly  different.  For 
I example,  there  is  full  fluorescent  lighting  on  two  aviaries;  on  one  aviary  they  receive  only  a comer 
1 of  supplemental  lighting  (all  fluorescent  lighting  is  set  to  go  on  from  0700  to  1730hrs.).  Aviary  1 


I 


Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5  231 


is  set  up  with  a live  potted  ficus  tree  and  a small  cup  nest  attached  within  the  branching.  Aviaries  4 
and  5 have  plastic  palm  trees  in  them  with  cup  nests.  A large  cup  nest  was  later  added  to  Aviary  5.  A 
covered  cup  nest  was  added  to  Aviary  4 later  in  the  season. 

General  Husbandry 

The  care  of  the  birds  was  basic.  Water  dishes  are 
changed  daily.  The  aviaries  are  hosed  twice  a 
week  from  the  outside  causing  a minimum  amount 
of  disturbance.  On  occasion  the  aviaries  require 
scrubbing;  in  these  cases  the  amount  of  time  spent  in 
the  aviary  is  limited  to  prevent  disturbing  the  birds. 

Their  diet  pan  is  offered  each  morning.  A pair  of 
bulbuls  receives  a pan  diet  of  varied  leafy  mix, 
frugivore  (chopped  up  fruit  and  vegetables  mixed 
with  soaked  Mazuri®  softbill  pellets.  In  the  a.m. 
mealworms  are  added  to  pan.  Waxworms  or  maggots 
are  added  to  the  pan  in  the  afternoon.  Once  chicks 
hatch,  additional  insects  are  offered  three  to  four  times  a day  and  include  cricket  abdomens. 

Breeding  and  Rearing 

In  general,  nesting  cups  were  placed  at  eye  level  to  limit  disturbance  when  nests  were  checked  for 
eggs.  Cup  nests  and  nesting  materials  were  added  on  19  February  2009.  Cotton,  pulled  burlap,  camel 
hair  and  untangled  twine  (about  4”  inches  long/10.16cm)  were  used  as  nesting  materials  It  appeared 
that  all  materials  offered  were  used  to  some  degree  in  the  nests.  All  nesting  attempts  were  made  using 
the  provided  nests.  By  4 March  2009  the  first  nests  were  complete  and  an  egg  was  discovered.  The 
last  chick  for  the  season  hatched  30  June  2009. 

Each  of  the  three  aviaries  had  more  than  one  clutch  hatch  and  all  chicks  fledged.  Aviary  1 fledging 
three  clutches.  All  chicks  were  parent  reared.  One  to  three  chicks  hatched  per  clutch.  A total  of  twelve 
chicks  were  successfully  hatched  and  fledged  in  a four-month  period. 

Challenges  Overcome 

In  Aviary  4 a covered  cup  nest  was  added  after  eggs 
disappeared  out  of  the  open  cup  nest.  This  was  done  in 
hopes  that  the  covered  nest  would  provide  better  security. 

This  worked  well  as  the  covered  nest  has  been  the  nest 
successfully  used  for  multiple  clutches  by  this  pair. 

Initially  we  had  problems  with  mice.  Along  with  improvement 
in  our  rodent  control  program  we  pulled  food  in  the  evening 
and  added  free-standing  platfomis  on  which  we  placed  the 
food  pans.  These  actions  decreased  the  rodent  population  and 
limited  their  access  to  the  food. 

Although  we  did  not  encounter  any  major  medical  issues 
during  the  breeding  season  we  did  encounter  a couple  of 
problems  with  the  offspring.  One  chick  was  euthanized 
due  to  severe  deformities  to  its  feet.  This  appeared  to  be  a 
genetic  problem  as  the  particular  pair  had  previously  fledged 
offspring  with  similar  complications.  Two  chicks  died  and 
tested  positive  for  atoxoplasmosis.  After  this  finding  all  of 
our  bulbuls  were  treated  for  this  parasite.  We  now  place 
many  of  our  passerines  on  an  extended  treatment  for  atoxo 


Example  of  nest  placement  with 
a chick  in  a cup  nest  just  prior  to 

fledging.  (Photo  by  Laurie  Morgan) 


Aviaries  in  Tropic  World-Asia  bird  holding 


(Photo  by  Laurie  Morgan) 


232  Animal  Keepers'  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5 


using  Toltrazuril®  during  the  breeding  season.  We  use  a 25  mg/ml  (2.5%)  stock  solution  and  add  3ml 
of  stock  solution  to  1 litre  of  water  (creating  a 300  mg/gal  solution).  This  dilution  is  used  as  the  only 
source  of  drinking  water.  Birds  are  to  be  treated  for  five  days  on,  three  days  off  Repeat  for  a total  of 
56  days.  This  treatment  starts  from  the  first  day  we  confirm  incubation  and  usually  lasts  for  several 
weeks  after  fledge. 

Findings 

The  following  items  may  have  contributed  to  the  overall  breeding  success: 

• As  a common  factor  all  three  aviaries  have  solid  sight  barriers  between  them.  These  panels 
provide  visual  baffles  so  the  pairs  can  hear  each  other  but  cannot  see  their  neighbors. 

• The  open  view  of  the  exhibit  (through  the  mesh  at  the  end  of  the  aviaries)  may  give  the  birds 
a feeling  of  huge  space  without  actually  having  access  to  it.  Coupled  with  the  echoing  effect 
of  their  calls,  this  may  convince  the  birds  that  their  territories  are  vast,  not  the  smaller  spaces 
in  which  they  are  actually  living.  The  echo  effect  may  also  convince  them  their  neighbors  are 
across  the  expanse,  not  next  door  to  them. 

• Since  each  aviary  was  set  up  slightly  different  and  all  three  pairs  produced  multiple  clutches, 
this  species  doesn’t  appear  to  be  overly  picky  about  cage  set-up. 

• In  our  experience  there  was  no  harassment  by  parents  or  their  siblings  once  chicks  fledged. 

In  Aviary  5 we  had  a successful  second  clutch  reared  with  previous  fledged  siblings  still  in 
the  same  aviary  (there  was  no  aggression  from  older  siblings  or  parents).  A total  of  six  birds 
are  currently  housed  in  that  aviary.  In  our  current  situation  we  have  been  able  to  keep  parents 
with  young  long  after  fledging,  although  historically  it  has  not  been  successful  due  to  the 
territorial  and  aggressive  nature  of  this  species. 

• Although  we  hatched  no  additional  chicks  after  30  June  there  is  no  way  to  know  if  this  was  due 
to  an  end  of  the  typical  breeding  season  or  if  the  presence  of  the  juvenile  birds  triggered  the 
parents  to  stop. 

Summary 

Initially  when  the  birds  were  first  set  up  we  were  not  expecting  to  achieve  the  great  success  that  we  did 
so  quickly.  This  species  is  noted  for  aggression  and  they  are  considered  very  territorial.  Often  parents 

will  abandon  their  chicks  or  attack  them 
once  they  fledge.  When  breeding  occurred 
and  the  parents  raised  their  young  without 
issues,  we  began  to  look  at  what  factors 
were  contributing  to  this  success.  The  birds 
have  minimal  disturbance  from  the  keepers. 
Once  chicks  have  hatched,  abundant 
insect  supplies  are  offered  to  discourage 
competition.  The  territories  appear  to  be 
unchallenged  by  using  the  visual  barriers 
even  though  the  pairs  can  hear  each  other. 
The  visual  and  auditory  access  to  the  larger 
exhibit  may  expand  their  perception  of  their 
territories.  It  is  notable  that  we  were  able  to 
I raise  these  birds  in  small  aviaries.  What  is  more  significant  is  that  we  are  still  able  to  keep  multiple 
I clutches  together  over  time. 

I 

I 

I As  of  4 January  2009,  nine  of  the  12  birds  that  fledged  survived;  some  are  still  being  housed  with 
i their  parents. 


Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5  233 


Sessions  for  All  Seasons:  Lappet-faced  Vulture  Training 


By  Eran  Brusilow,  Tricia  Emrich,  Tara  Lee  and  Kevin  McKay 
Animal  Keepers  at  Disney’s  Animal  Kingdom,  Lake  Buena  Vista,  EL 


Introduction 

Every  training  program  carries  its  own  unique  set  of  challenges.  The  Lappet-faced  Vulture  {Torgos 
tracheliotus)  training  program  at  Disney’s  Animal  Kingdom  is  no  exception.  The  success  we  enjoy 
as  a training  team  is  directly  related  to  our  ability  to  juggle  a multitude  of  distractions  - an  open- 
air  enclosure  that  is  subject  to  changing  weather  conditions,  the  constant  intrusion  of  pesky  native 
species,  the  presence  of  inquisitive  collection  animals  that  share  the  vultures’  habitat,  the  booming 
parades,  and  intraspecific  interactions  associated  with  courtship  and  breeding.  Vulture  training 
sessions  certainly  keep  all  of  the  trainers  on  their  toes  and  challenge  their  creativity. 

Our  Vultures 

Disney’s  Animal  Kingdom  is  home  to  two  (1.1)  rehabilitated  adult  Lappet-faced  vultures  that  were 
deemed  unfit  for  release  back  into  the  wild.  The  male  and  female  were  added  to  our  collection  in  2004 
at  the  ages  of  six  years  and  four  years,  respectively.  Their  enclosure  spans  approximately  6,500sq. 
ft.  [603.86sq  m]  and  includes  rock  wall  boundaries,  a shallow  waterway,  felled  timber  piles,  lush 
arbors  and  expansive  open  areas.  The  vultures  share  this  enclosure  with  Red  Kangaroos  {Macropus 
rufus),  West  African  Black  Crowned  Cranes  {Balearica  pavonia).  Lesser  Flamingos  (Phoenicopterus 
minor)  and  White-faced  Whistling  Ducks  (Dendrocygna  viduata).  Feedings/training  sessions  usually 
occur  once  daily  in  the  afternoon  and  roughly  300  grams  [10.58  oz.]  of  food  are  distributed  to  each 
bird.  Currently,  there  is  no  off-exhibit  holding  area  in  which  to  train,  so  these  sessions  are  performed 
within  the  vultures’  mixed-species  habitat  and  always  in  full  view  of  our  guests. 

Training  Challenges 

The  vulture  training  program  exemplifies  Disney’s  well-established  commitment  to  behavioral 
husbandry.  Developing  a successful  program  for  our  birds  has  been  extremely  rewarding,  but  it  has 
also  required  us  to  creatively  troubleshoot  many  situations.  Integrating  new  keepers  and  balancing 
training  goals  with  breeding  goals  have  been  two  of  the  most  significant  challenges. 

Maintaining  consistency  within  our  program  is  critical.  This  was  first  achieved  in  early  2009  by 
enlisting  four  zookeepers,  with  overlapping  schedules,  who  would  be  responsible  for  developing 
and  implementing  specific  training  strategies.  The  availability  of  vulture  training  team  members 
throughout  the  week  facilitates  improved  communication  between  trainers,  behavioral  husbandry 
specialists,  zoological  managers  and  other  zookeepers.  Also,  brainstorming  and  problem-solving 
exercises  are  generally  more  productive  when  a larger  number  of  people  participate.  While  it  seems 
that  four  zookeepers  overseeing  the  training  of  two  birds  is  undoubtedly  a recipe  for  success,  multiple 
zookeepers  training  a single  vulture  can  also  create  an  atmosphere  of  inconsistency.  Variation  may 
exist  in  a vulture’s  response  to  the  different  members  of  the  training  team.  Attempts  to  combat  this 
phenomenon  are  made  by  constantly  rotating  trainers.  As  a consequence,  it  is  possible  that  one 
vulture  may  interact  with  four  different  trainers  across  four  consecutive  days.  Hence,  it  is  imperative 
that  the  cues  established  for  particular  behaviors  be  performed  similarly  by  all  trainers. 

We  are  also  committed  to  breeding  our  vultures.  The  future  propagation  of  this  species  can  be  greatly 
enhanced  by  incorporating  a training  program.  However,  our  training  progress  has  been  adversely 
affected  by  factors  that  dramatically  alter  the  behavior  of  our  vultures.  Elevated  hormone  levels 
and  various  environmental  stimuli  associated  with  the  onset  of  breeding  season  (winter  and  spring) 
cause  aggressive  responses.  The  vultures  may  respond  aggressively  toward  approaching  trainers  in 
attempts  to  defend  their  nesting  territory.  Mate  aggression  is  also  frequently  observed  as  the  birds 
vie  for  space  or  food.  The  size  and  speed  of  these  vultures  can  make  negative  responses  like  these, 
extremely  intimidating  for  nearby  trainers. 


234  Animal  Keepers’ Eorum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5 


Meeting  the  Challenges 

Within  their  first  year  at  Disney’s  Animal  Kingdom,  the  vultures  were  trained  to  station  on  large  logs 
that  measure  15  inches  in  diameter,  20  inches  in  height  and  are  situated  15  feet  from  one  another 
[38cm  in  diameter,  50.8cm  high,  and  4.57m  apart].  Upon  receiving  a visual  “station”  cue  and  verbal 
“go  on”,  they  were  reinforced  for  mounting  the  stations  and  displaying  a relaxed  posture.  Feeding 
was  often  done  by  a single  trainer,  from  a moderate  distance  and  over  a relatively  short  time  period. 
Several  attempts  were  made  to  advance  these  rudimentary  behaviors  over  the  next  four  years, 
however  progress  proved  difficult. 


Figure  1.  Tara  reinforces  “Bones”  on  his  station.  (Photo  by  Erm  BrusUow) 

During  the  spring  of  2009,  in  the  wake  of  the  breeding  season  mayhem,  changes  were  made  to  usher 
in  a new  era  of  vulture  training.  The  first  change  involved  restructuring  the  core  training  team  to 
include  four  individuals  who  were  enthusiastic  about  working  with  the  vultures.  Feeding  could  now 
be  performed  by  two  trainers  on  a daily  basis.  Secondly,  we  reduced  the  feeding  distance  to  a few 
feet  (at  the  bird’s  discretion)  and  the  number  of  reinforcements  was  increased  in  order  to  lengthen 
training  sessions.  Although  the  vultures  appeared  to  take  well  to  having  their  own  trainers,  they  were 
still  able  to  see  one  another  throughout  the  sessions.  This  meant  that  the  birds  would  often  disengage 
from  their  own  sessions  to  investigate  the  other  trainer,  the  other  vulture,  a dropped  meatball,  etc. 
To  combat  these  sudden  breaks  in  concentration,  we  began  to  reinforce  the  vultures  for  rotating  on 
their  stations  as  we  walked  around  them.  Eventually,  the  birds  rotated  so  that  they  faced  opposite 
directions.  We  wanted  to  create  two  independent  training  sessions  that  happen  to  occur  at  the  same 
time  and  in  the  same  immediate  area  (Fig.  2). 


Figure  2.  Kevin  and  Tara  working  vultures  independently.  (Phow  by  Patrida  Emrich) 
Encouraged  by  the  vultures’  rapid  progress,  we  compiled  a short  list  of  other  behaviors  that  we 


Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5  235 


deemed  essential  for  improving  their  care.  Training  the  vultures  to  stand  on  a scale  platform  seemed 
to  be  a natural  progression.  We  constructed  two  durable  scale  covers  topped  with  thick  perches, 
which  fit  snuggly  over  a wireless  scale.  These  scale  covers  were  introduced  into  the  enclosure  over 
the  course  of  a couple  weeks  during  training  sessions  and  were  eventually  placed  a few  feet  from  the 
stations  without  the  vultures  reacting  negatively.  When  we  first  began  shaping  the  “scale”  behavior, 
the  trainers  gave  a visual  “scale”  cue,  said  “scale”  and  immediately  enticed  the  vultures  off  of  the 
stations  in  the  direction  of  the  scale  covers.  Following  the  “scale”  cues,  golf  ball-sized  meatballs 
would  be  rapidly  tossed  at  14,  14,  and  % of  the  distance  from  the  station  to  the  scale  cover.  As  the 
vultures  ate  the  food  items  closest  to  the  scale  covers,  they  would  be  asked  to  station  again.  Once 
the  birds  began  responding  to  the  “scale”  cues  before  the  first  meatball  was  thrown,  we  decreased  the 
number  of  reinforcements  along  the  path  to  the  scale  cover  and  increased  the  amount  of  time  between 
each  reinforcement.  Eventually,  we  performed  the  “scale”  cues  and  only  reinforced  a vulture  for 
placing  a foot  on  the  scale  cover.  Our  female  was  the  first  of  the  two  birds  to  receive  the  “scale”  cue, 
dismount  her  station,  walk  calmly  toward  the  apparatus  and  step  atop  the  scale  cover.  Less  than  one 
week  later,  the  wireless  scale  was  placed  under  the  scale  cover  and  her  first  ever  voluntary  weight 
was  recorded. 


Although  the  “scale”  behavior  was  a tremendous  accomplishment  for  our  training  team,  the  vultures 
were  developing  a bad  habit.  They  began  to  jump  from  either  the  station  or  scale  to  catch  food  items 
that  were  tossed  to  them.  Such  undesirable  behavior  could  cause  injury  to  the  birds  and  the  trainers, 
so  we  agreed  that  hand-feeding  was 
while  wearing  thick  leather  gloves 
on  our  left  arms.  The  habituation 
period  was  surprisingly  brief  and 
both  birds  did  not  seem  the  least 
bit  unsettled  at  the  sight  of  the 
awkward  gloves.  When  the  birds 
were  stationed,  trainers  would  place 
a meatball  between  their  left  index 
and  middle  fingers,  hold  a second 
meatball  in  their  right  hand,  extend 
the  gloved  hand  toward  the  bird  and 
reinforce  them  for  remaining  calm 
by  tossing  the  meatball  from  the 
right  hand.  After  a few  days,  the 
gloved  hand  could  be  extended  so 
closely  to  the  beak  that  we  simply 
allowed  the  vulture  to  gently  pluck 
the  meatball  from  between  our 
gloved  fingers  (Fig.  3). 

Figure  3.  Kevin  hand-feeds  a calm  “Carri.” 

We  also  introduced  the  vultures  to  a 
“heel”  behavior,  in  which  they  are 

asked  to  calmly  follow  their  trainers  throughout  the  enclosure.  They  were  originally  baited  off  of 
their  stations  after  a visual  “heel”  cue  was  demonstrated  by  the  trainers.  We  would  cue,  drop  a 
meatball  at  our  feet,  back  up  a short  distance,  drop  another  meatball  at  our  feet,  back  up  a short 
distance,  etc.  One  important  point  to  mention  is  that  we  tried  to  keep  the  vultures  moving  and 
always  finished  the  “heel”  behavior  back  on  the  station.  Gobbling  down  a small  meatball  takes  mere 
seconds,  so  the  trainers  had  to  be  ready  with  reinforcements  and  back-up  plans  to  keep  the  birds 
in  tow.  Similar  to  our  “scale”  behavior  progressions,  the  birds  eventually  began  to  dismount  their 
stations  immediately  after  they  received  the  visual  “heel”  cue.  Baiting  was  then  phased  out  and  the 
birds  were  not  reinforced  until  they  arrived  close  enough  to  trainers  to  be  hand-fed. 


There  are  instances  in  which  our  vultures  will  extend  one  or  both  wings  during  a training  session;  to 
balance  in  breezy  conditions  or  as  a response  to  a dive-bombing  red- shouldered  hawk,  for  example. 
We  seized  these  opportunities  to  capture  a “wing  present”  behavior  by  extending  our  hands  to  mirror 
those  of  the  birds,  bridging  and  immediately  reinforcing  their  behavior  (Fig.  4). 


236  Animal  Keepers'  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5 


Avian  Trivia.  . . 

Why  don’t  perching  birds  fall  out  of  trees  when  they  sleep?  When  perching  birds  sit, 
a tendon  on  the  backside  of  the  ankle  automatically  flexes  locking  their  toes  around  the 
branch.  With  feet  locked,  sleeping  birds  don’t  fall.  As  the  bird  stands  up,  its  feet  release. 

• There  are  109  species  of  birds  that  have  become  extinct.  • 90%  of  all  species  that  have  become 
extinct  have  been  birds.  • The  maximum  speed  of  a chicken  is  nine  miles  per  hour.  It  takes  40 
minutes  to  hard-boil  an  ostrich  egg.  • Feathers  make  up  10%  of  a bird’s  total  body  weight.  • If  an 
average  man  had  a metabolism  comparable  to  that  of  a hummingbird  he  would  have  to  eat  285 
pounds  of  hamburger  every  day  to  maintain  his  weight.  • The  hummingbird  is  the  only  bird  that  can 
fly  backwards.  • The  pouch  of  a pelican  has  a capacity  to  carry  12  quarts.  • New  York  City  has  the 
largest  population  of  peregrine  falcons  in  the  world.  • The  average  life  span  of  a robin  is  12  years.  • 
The  nest  of  a bald  eagle  can  be  12  feet  deep,  10  feet  wide  and  weigh  over  a ton.  • Birds  actually  dream 
when  they  sleep.  According  to  research  conducted  by  the  University  of  Chicago,  they  dream  about 
their  songs,  have  dream  rehearsals,  and  sing  the  same  songs  better  the  next  day.  • All  of  the  swans  in 
England  are  the  property  of  the  queen.  Disturbing  or  bothering  them  is  considered  a serious  offense.  • 
An  ostrich’s  eye  is  twice  as  large  as  its  brain  and  weighs  3.3  pounds.  • Frigate  birds  can  fly  at  a speed 
of  260  miles  per  hour.  • The  American  crow  is  found  in  every  USA  state  except  Hawaii. 

Source : http ://www.cockatielcottage  .net/ trivia  .html 


Looking  Ahead 

When  breeding  season  arrives,  we  do  not  introduce  new  behaviors  until  the  birds  exhibit  normal 
behavior.  We  do,  however,  continue  with  their  daily  training  regimen.  The  vultures  frequently 
cooperate  during  training  sessions,  albeit  less  reliably.  The  hope  is  to  simply  maintain  learned 

behaviors  throughout  the  breeding  season, 
after  which  we  will  usher  in  new  training 
strategies.  For  example,  we  hope  to  build 
upon  the  “heel”  behavior  so  that  the  vultures 
shift  voluntarily  into  a small  containment 
annex.  This  is  essential  because  landscaping 
tasks  occasionally  need  to  be  conducted 
inside  the  habitat.  In  these  instances,  the 
vultures  are  corralled  into  the  containment 
area  by  a large  number  of  zookeepers, 
until  the  work  is  finished;  a very  negative 
experience  for  all  involved.  We  also  plan  to 
expand  the  “wing  present”  behavior  in  order 
to  regularly  perform  feather  trims.  Trainers 
will  then  be  able  to  diminish  flight  capability, 
without  restraining  the  birds. 


Earlier  experiences  with  the  vultures  led  us 
Figure  4.  Kevin  performing  the  “wing  present”  cue.  to  believe  that  training  during  the  breeding 
(Photo  by  Patricia  Emrich)  season  wouM  be  impossible.  Aggressive  and 

uncooperative  behaviors  can  manifest  for 
up  to  six  months  of  each  year,  cutting  our  effective  training  window  in  half.  Despite  these  major 
challenges,  we  have  had  much  success  in  maintaining  positive  training  sessions  by  putting  together 
an  enthusiastic  team,  creating  a solid  training  plan  and  communicating  with  each  other.  Our  first 
four  years  of  vulture  training  were  marked  by  plenty  of  trial  and  error,  learning  what  works  for  the 
vultures  as  well  as  the  trainers.  Persistence  was  key,  not  giving  up  when  our  methods  did  not  produce 
the  desired  results.  All  of  our  training  goals  are  by  no  means  complete  but  our  commitment  to  the 
program  has  allowed  us  to  accomplish  a great  deal  in  this  last  year.  As  the  program  continues  to 
evolve  we  are  positioned  to  seamlessly  transition  from  one  season  to  the  next,  building  upon  past 
successes. 


Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5  237 


Successfully  Managing  a Mixed-Species,  Free-Flight  Aviary 


By  Paula  Kolvig,  Assistant  Curator;  Heather  Leeson,  Biologist  III; 
Jeana  McDowell  and  Chris  St.  Romain,  Biologists  I 
Moody  Gardens,  Galveston,  TX 


Rainforest  Exhibit 

Moody  Gardens  is  a non-profit  organization  which  boasts  lush  landscaped  grounds,  IMAX®  theaters, 
restaurants,  a hotel,  convention  center,  and  two  main  live  animal  exhibits:  the  Rainforest  Pyramid 
and  the  Aquarium  Pyramid.  The  Rainforest  Pyramid  exhibit  at  Moody  Gardens  opened  in  1993. 
The  original  scope  of  the  exhibit  had  been  planned  as  a botanical  garden  with  a few  live  animals  for 
enhancement.  However,  within  the  first  year,  the  focus  changed  to  allow  equal  emphasis  on  animal 
and  plant  diversity  found  in  the  rainforest  ecosystem.  This  exhibit,  which  covers  one  square  acre,  is 
a ten-story  glass  pyramid  that  depicts  the  diversity 


The  collection  plan  evolved  and  developed 
through  the  years  and  culminated  in  2008  as  an 
exhibit  with  a wide  variety  of  taxa  and  species 
housed  together  in  this  realistic  rainforest  setting. 
A diverse  representation  of  tropical  birds,  fishes, 
amphibians,  reptiles,  and  small  mammals  made 
up  the  animal  component  of  the  Rainforest 
exhibit.  Each  new  exhibit  enhancement  over  the 
previous  years  brought  on  a focus  of  the  different 
taxa  of  animals  listed  above.  Every  third  year 
or  so,  the  focus  would  change  and  new  animals 
were  brought  into  the  mix.  The  animal  inventory 
in  September  of  2008  was  roughly  over  1,000 
animals  of  six  different  taxa. 


The  Rainforest  Pyramid  at  Moody  Gardens. 


(Photo  by  Heather  Leeson) 


This  paper  will  cover  the  avian  species  and 

management  of  the  exhibit  up  until  September  2008.  Hurricane  Ike  hit  Galveston,  where  Moody 
Gardens  is  located,  on  13  September  2008.  Due  to  the  extent  of  damage  from  the  storm  surge, 
surviving  animals  from  the  Rainforest  collection  were  relocated  to  many  AZA  facilities.  The 
evacuation  began  on  1 7 September  2008  and,  to  some  degree,  relocation  is  still  underway.  Over  800 
animals  were  moved  to  zoos  and  aquariums  within  a three-month  period.  While  some  of  the  animals 
have  since  been  brought  back  to  Moody  Gardens,  the  majority  remains  out  on  emergency  loan  until 
the  Rainforest  repair  and  renovation  work  can  be  completed. 


Since  the  exhibit  was  intended  to  be  an  immersive  experience  for  the  visitor,  animals  were  free- 
roaming  or  free-flighted  as  much  as  possible.  Although  this  paper  will  focus  on  the  avian  species, 
a brief  mention  of  other  animals  that  were  free-roaming  gives  a clearer  picture  of  the  exhibit’s 
complexity.  Free-ranging  mammals  included  Cotton-top  Tamarins  (Saguinus  oedipus)  and  Two-toed 
Sloths  {Choloepus  didactylus).  Of  these,  the  tamarins  were  trained  to  recall  and  managed  strictly  for 
behaviors.  The  sloths  were  fed  from  particular  stations  and  some  training  was  successful,  but  they 
were  not  recall  trained. 


Reptiles  that  could  be  found  free-roaming  through  the  exhibit  were  Sailfin  Lizards  (Hydrosaurus 
amboinensis)',  Tokay  Geckos  {Gekko  gecko)',  Asian  Water  Dragons  (Physignathus  cocincinus), 
Basilisks  {Basiliscus  plumifrons  and  B.  vittatus).  Day  Geckos  (Phelsuma  species)  and  historically 
Green  Iguanas  {Iguana  iguana).  Of  these  lizard  species,  all  but  the  Tokay  geckos  were  recall  and 
target-trained  to  better  manage  their  care.  The  larger  lizard  species  were  consistently  counted,  fed, 
and  managed;  whereas  some  of  the  smaller  species,  like  the  young  basilisks,  were  identified  and 
trained  as  often  as  they  presented  themselves. 


238  Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5 


A few  amphibian  species  were  also  exhibited  as  free  roaming  in  the  Rainforest  Pyramid.  Species 
displayed  included  Green  Tree  Frogs  {Hyla  cinerea).  Squirrel  Tree  Frogs  (Hyla  squire  Ha),  as  well 
as  historical  attempts  at  displaying  Poison  Dart  Frogs  (Dendrobates  species),  and  Bullfrogs  (Rana 
catesbeiana).  The  Poison  dart  frogs  were  an  unsuccessful  species  in  the  collection  due  to  predation 
by  birds.  The  Bullfrogs  were  eventually  phased  out  of  the  collection  due  to  predation  by  fish  and 
birds  when  they  were  small;  in  addition,  as  they  grew  larger,  there  was  concern  of  bullfrog  predation 
on  small  birds  and  fish.  Behavior  management  was  not  attempted  on  any  amphibian  species. 

The  Rainforest  Pyramid  also  included  an  outdoor  waterfowl  exhibit.  This  open-top  exhibit  was 
viewed  by  windows  and  did  not  have  public  access.  The  50,000-gallon  shallow  pond  with  waterfalls 
featured  various  waterfowl,  pelicans,  flamingos,  as  well  as  one  mammal  species.  Red-flanked  Duiker 
(Cephalophus  rufilatus).  All  of  the  birds  in  this  exhibit  were  flight  restricted  either  by  pinioning 
or  wing  clipping.  The  pelicans  (and  historically  a Magnificent  Frigate)  were  rehabilitated,  non- 
releasable  with  various  wing  injuries  that  restricted  flight.  The  land  to  water  ratio  was  approximately 
60%  water  to  40%  land,  and  the  space  was  utilized  for  holding  of  some  tortoises  as  well.  Some 
behavior  management  was  underway  on  a few  species  of  both  taxa. 

Avian  Collection 

The  majority  of  the  avian  species  were  free-flighted;  a few  were  free-roaming  due  to  flight  restriction 
by  pinioning.  By  September  2008,  the  Rainforest  was  home  to  a variety  of  tropical  pigeons,  doves, 
turacos,  waterfowl,  small  psittacines,  ibis,  oropendolas,  rollers,  and  finches  to  name  a few.  Table  1 
lists  the  current  avian  collection.  The  larger  psittacines  are  housed  in  off-exhibit  holding  and  perch- 
trained.  They  are  perched  throughout  the  rainforest  for  several  hours  during  the  day  and  their  wings 
are  clipped  on  a regular  basis. 

As  the  avian  collection  developed  and  changed  over  time,  new  species  were  added  or  phased  out 
depending  on  the  how  well  they  adapted  to  the  exhibit.  As  much  as  the  size  and  complexity  of 
the  exhibit  provided  opportunities  for  breeding  and  behavior  management,  the  exhibit  also  created 
challenges  for  captive  avian  management.  The  main  challenge  to  successful  management  was  having 
a variety  of  birds  with  different  niches  under  stimulus  control.  The  exhibit  size,  while  a bonus  to 
many  birds,  proved  difficult  for  staff  to  get  the  free  flighted  birds  where  we  needed  them  when  we 
needed  them.  Nests  were  also  easy  to  miss  if  they  were  high  enough  in  the  canopy  of  the  exhibit  and 
access  to  these  nests  was  restricted  as  well. 

Avian  Diets 

As  mentioned  above,  managing  a diverse  free-flight  aviary  is  a challenge.  With  a diverse  collection 
of  birds  comes  a diverse  set  of  nutritional  needs  and  requirements.  Our  primary  goal  is  to  meet  those 
needs  while  maintaining  healthy  weights  on  a variety  of  animals.  With  the  provision  of  a diverse 
array  of  food  items,  feeding  methods,  and  monitoring,  the  proper  nutrition  for  each  bird  group  can 
be  achieved. 

The  Rainforest  staff  utilized  a large  amount  of  food  items  for  the  aviary  that  range  from  specialty 
items  like  formulated  dry  food  items  to  fresh  produce  and  some  higher  protein  sources.  The  majority 
of  the  formulated  diets,  which  are  commonly  used  in  the  zoological  setting,  include  items  from 
Mazuri®  diets  (Parrot,  Flamingo,  Softbill,  etc).  In  addition,  a variety  of  seeds  and  grains  were  offered. 
Supplements,  such  as  vitamins  and  bird  gel,  were  added  to  the  diets  to  ensure  that  picky  eaters  were 
receiving  adequate  nutrition  even  if  they  picked  around  the  formulated  foods.  A variety  of  fresh 
fruits  and  vegetables  were  offered  differing  in  sizes,  amounts,  and  types.  Items  such  as  apples  were 
a staple  for  any  fruit  eater,  and  depending  on  the  type  of  bird,  could  be  presented  in  chopped,  halved, 
or  finely  diced  form.  This  was  the  case  for  all  produce  offered.  Rotating  the  types  of  fruit  and 
vegetables  allowed  for  better  enrichment  and  nutritional  opportunities. 

Utilizing  different  feeding  methods  allowed  the  staff  to  accommodate  for  all  of  the  bird  types 
while  creating  a positive  guest  experience.  Two  different  types  of  feeding  methods  were  primarily 
employed  within  the  Rainforest:  feed  stations  and  free  feeding.  Height,  location,  quantity,  and 
feeding  preferences  were  some  of  the  considerations  taken  into  account  with  the  presentation  of 
food.  Feed  stations  were  either  stationary  (permanent)  or  mobile  and  situated  along  the  trail,  out 
of  reach  of  guests.  The  four  permanent  stations  allowed  for  the  placement  of  a food  pan  within  a 


Animal  Keepers'  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5  239 


concrete  tree  stump  feeder.  The  first  feeder  at 
the  entry  of  the  Rainforest  was  approximately 
at  eye  level,  placed  further  within  the  foliage, 
and  inaccessible  to  guests,  yet  allowed  them 
to  see  the  birds  feeding.  The  next  two  feeders 
were  stationed  about  shoulder  height,  within 
the  view  of  the  guests,  and  below  small  rock 
ledges.  The  final  feeder  was  at  ground  level  and 
semi-secluded  by  foliage  off  the  pathway.  A 
fifth  pan  was  regularly  placed  in  a howdy  cage 
to  acclimate  birds  to  being  fed  inside,  thereby 
allowing  staff  to  easily  capture  untrained  birds 
out  of  the  Rainforest  as  necessary. 

The  free-roaming  waterfowl  had  originally 
been  fed  at  various  ponds  throughout  the 
exhibit,  according  to  their  range.  For  example, 
the  Hottentot  ducks  were  fed  from  a pan  in  a stream  located  on  the  African  trail  of  the  exhibit. 
Throughout  the  years,  the  waterfowl  that  were  not  contained  within  their  particular  ranges  began  to 
show  up  at  one  particular  pond  each  morning  for  feeding.  It  was  not  determined  whether  this  was  due 
to  competition,  a preference  of  food  presentation  and  location,  or  type  of  food  pan.  Due  to  this  fact, 
the  feeding  for  the  various  waterfowl  in  the  Rainforest  changed  to  a broadcast  feed  in  the  “ibis”  pond 
on  the  South  American  trail.  This  was  one  example  as  to  how  the  animals  acclimated  over  time  and 
feeding  techniques  were  adapted  to  their  needs.  The  flock  of  Scarlet  ibis  was  fed  from  a stationary 
feed  pan  in  their  pond,  and  the  waterfowl  periodically  shared  feeding  times  and  food  with  the  ibis. 
Mobile  feed  stations  consisted  of  feeder  logs,  such  as  bamboo  feeders  with  branches  for  piercing 
fruit  and  vegetables,  or  log  feeders  that  contained  holes  for  the  placement  of  food  items.  Staff-made 
bamboo  feeders  can  easily  be  constructed  out  of  a log  and  nails  that  are  used  to  pierce  the  food.  These 
feeders  can  be  easily  removed,  washed,  and  sanitized. 

Free  feeding  occurred  along  the  trail  as  the  food  cart  was  taken  into  the  exhibit.  This  also  allowed 
for  the  best  observation  time  for  staff  Birds  often  followed  the  food  cart  and  over  time,  the  birds  that 
were  more  acclimated  and  brave  would  ride  on  the  food  cart  helping  themselves  to  the  buffet  before 
them.  This  bird  behavior  afforded  the  staff  great  training  and  enrichment  opportunities. 

The  feeding  methods  discussed  helped  staff  to  monitor  the  birds  through  observations  and  training. 
Many  birds  would  visit  particular  stations  in  a known  pattern  and  time.  Some  were  more  random 
with  their  feeding  preferences,  but  each  feeding  opportunity  became  a management  opportunity  for 
the  staff  The  exhibit  opened  at  lOOOhrs  and  the  first  feeding  would  occur  close  to  opening  time. 
This  consistency  allowed  guests  to  catch  a glimpse  of  the  majority  of  birds  in  the  collections,  as  well 
as  their  feeding  habits  and  natural  behaviors.  The  morning  feed  was  an  opportune  time  for  training 
sessions  and  was  used  as  a tool  to  passively  condition  the  birds  to  a food  station.  Longer  business 
hours  during  the  summer  allowed  for  more  feedings  and  training  sessions.  Regardless  of  the  season 
or  business  hours,  the  food  pans  were  available  to  the  birds  for  a minimum  of  seven  hours.  This  was 
done  for  pest  control  reasons  and  to  allow  the  more  timid  birds  to  find  food  and  forage  throughout  the 
day,  as  it  was  typically  the  bolder  species  that  had  first  dibs  on  food. 

During  the  breeding  and  nesting  season  animals  expend  more  energy  and  some  may  require  increased 
protein  intake.  In  order  to  accommodate  for  this  change  in  energy  demand,  normal  diets  were 
supplemented  by  incorporating  items  such  as  formulated  breeder  grains,  live  invertebrates,  and  live 
rodent  pinkies.  Some  species  were  target-fed  these  items;  for  example,  the  Blue-bellied  rollers  were 
quite  capable  of  catching  items  mid-flight  and  the  Sunbittem  was  acclimated  to  taking  crickets  off 
feeder  tongs.  Other  supplements  were  added  into  all  of  the  pans  since  the  birds  had  free  choice  of 
where  to  eat.  The  amounts  were  monitored  and  adjusted  as  needed.  Through  the  years,  many  birds 
were  acclimated  to  hand  feeding.  While  this  technique  ensured  a certain  bird  received  a certain  item, 
it  was  only  used  sporadically  and  with  caution,  as  the  staff  did  not  want  a constant  association  with 
human  fingers  and  food.  In  a free-flight  situation  where  the  public  has  contact  with  so  many  animals, 
this  can  be  startling  to  the  unsuspecting  guest  pointing  at  something. 


240  Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5 


Food  presentation  and  distribution  encouraged  equal  feeding  and  enrichment  opportunities. 
Occasionally  the  food  wasn’t  mixed  up  but  was  presented  in  distinct  sections  of  the  pan,  or  was 
chopped  differently  for  enrichment.  Other  ways  to  meet  the  various  needs  were  to  alternate  soaking 
pelleted  foods  with  water  and/or  diluted  fruit  juice,  cooking  some  of  the  vegetables  periodically,  and 
rotating  in  some  seasonal  produce  items.  Enrichment  was  also  offered  in  the  form  of  live  invertebrates 
(crickets,  worms,  roaches  etc),  spray  millet,  or  alternating  different  seeds. 

The  exhibit  itself  provided  ample  enrichment  for  the  animals  (of  all  taxa),  but  especially  for  many  of 
the  birds.  Many  fruit  bearing  trees  produced  plenty  of  food  for  the  birds  to  snack  on.  For  example,  at 
least  three  types  of  ficus  produced  figs  and  many  of  the  palm  fruits  were  available  year  round.  Other 
types  of  fruit  that  grew  in  the  exhibit  were  two  types  of  citrus,  banana,  and  star  fruit  (which  was  a 
staff  favorite  as  well).  In  addition  to  the  naturally  occurring  horticulture  enrichment,  a variety  of  bugs 
were  naturally  available.  These  additional  food  items  afforded  many  natural  avian  behaviors  which 
was  a great  guest  experience  but  a challenging  husbandry  issue.  Birds  that  are  feeding  themselves 
at  the  top  of  the  canopy  have  very  little  incentive  to  come  to  a keeper  no  matter  how  many  times  a 
favorite  food  item  is  presented.  This  created  yet  another  challenge  to  the  overall  management  of  the 
animals  and  exhibit  at  Moody  Gardens. 

Avian  Breeding 

Since  the  Rainforest  opened  in  1993,  we  have  housed  84  species  of  breeding  birds;  of  those,  41  species 
have  successfully  bred!  That’s  a 48%  success  rate  of  which  we  are  very  proud.  These  included  birds 
that  live  in  pairs  or  groups  of  three  or  more.  Some  of  our  paired  birds  included:  Yellow-breasted 
ground  doves,  Coscoroba  swans.  Crested  oropendola.  Fairy  bluebird.  Peacock  pheasant.  Pink  pigeon. 
Piping  guan,  and  Shelducks.  Some  examples  of  group  birds  are  Finches,  Nicobar  pigeons.  Speckled 
pigeons.  Collared  doves.  Scarlet  ibis.  Pintails,  and  Crested  wood  partridge. 

The  location  of  a nest  can  tell  keepers  a lot,  most  importantly  where  the  nest  is,  which  is  challenging 
to  find  in  a lush  and  heavily  planted  immense  exhibit.  By  knowing  the  location,  keepers  can  tally 
approximate  hatch  dates,  keep  watch  for  chicks  around  that  date,  monitor  behavior  of  parents,  and 
coordinate  with  the  horticulture  staff  so  watering  of  plants  in  that  area  can  be  done  cautiously. 

There  are  infinite  places  to  build  a nest  and  a variety  of  birds  that  build  nests  in  different  niches. 
Due  to  the  sheer  vastness  of  the  Rainforest  it  is  impossible  to  account  for  every  nest.  No  material 
is  provided,  aside  from  what  occurs  naturally  in  the  exhibit.  Historically,  nest  material  such  as 
excelsior  was  provided,  but  was  not  utilized  by  the  bids.  Also,  supplemental  commercial  finch  nests 
were  hung  around  the  exhibit,  but  again,  the  birds  that  were  successful  built  their  own  from  naturally 
occurring  materials.  The  nests  in  the  Rainforest  consisted  mainly  of  sticks,  leaves,  air  roots,  and 
curtain  ivy.  Many  nests  weren’t  noticed  by  staff,  and  successful  breeding  was  only  noted  when  a 
chick  was  found  hanging  around  one  or  both  parents.  Surprise!  For  many  birds  such  as  finches,  fairy 
bluebirds,  pigeons,  and  doves  this  was  a common  occurrence.  For  other  birds,  nest  locations  were 
more  consistent  and  predictable. 

The  Scarlet  ibis  usually  nested  in  the  fall  in  the  highest  trees  around  the  largest  water  feature,  making 
for  very  precarious  chick  exploration.  The  Yellow-breasted  ground  doves  and  Palawan  peacock 
pheasant  chose  new  locations  for  each  ground  nest,  which  were  always  viewable  from  the  pathway. 
The  Crested  oropendola ’s  nest  is  quite  obvious  and  our  group  built  theirs  suspended  from  high 
canopy  points  in  the  center  of  the  exhibit.  Piping  guans  built  their  nests  approximately  every  three 
months  in  the  African  region  of  the  exhibit  next  to  ledges  facing  the  behind-the-scenes  area.  These 
nests  were  furthest  from  public  view  and  difficult  to  access.  The  Pink  pigeons  preferred  the  densely- 
covered  ficus  trees  for  each  nest.  The  outdoor  flamingo  pond  exhibit  had  many  nesting  opportunities 
as  well.  Frequently,  waterfowl  nest  boxes  were  placed  along  various  points  of  the  landward  side  of 
the  exhibit.  The  Coscoroba  swans  successfully  produced  cygnets  each  season,  and  their  nest  was  in 
the  same  general  location  each  time. 

Nesting  behavior  is  quite  intriguing.  Some  males  are  extremely  territorial,  such  as  the  swans  and 
the  piping  guan,  which  would  almost  require  keeper  protective  equipment  when  servicing  the  area. 
The  ibis  flock  would  cease  coming  down  to  eat  while  only  one  or  two  nests  were  being  tended  to. 
The  pair  of  Yellow-breasted  ground  doves  would  take  turns  sitting  the  nest,  while  the  male  Peacock 


Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5  241 


pheasant  would  not  only  refuse  nesting  duties  but  would  continue  to  court  his  mate  while  she  was 
sitting  the  nest.  Once  a nest  site  had  been  chosen,  there  was  nothing  keepers  could  do  to  change  its 
location.  A very  determined  pair  of  Speckled  pigeons  would  continuously  roost  on  slanted  rockwork 
above  the  exit  pathway.  Each  month,  an  undeveloped  egg  would  be  found  broken  on  the  pathway. . . 
evolution  at  its  best. 

Some  birds  were  excellent  breeders  in  the  exhibit;  others  never  quite  figured  it  out  or  appeared 
to  not  acknowledge  the  presence  of  a con-specific  mate  in  the  exhibit.  Six  species  were  constant 
contributors  to  the  genetic  pool  for  their  captive  populations:  Palawan  peacock  pheasant,  Coscoroba 
swan.  Piping  guan.  Speckled  pigeon.  Collared  dove,  and  many  waterfowl  species.  Often  times, 
we  were  faced  with  overpopulation  for  our  exhibit.  Finding  homes  for  many  of  these  species  can 
be  difficult  and  interest  seemed  to  be  cyclical.  The  Piping  guan  pair  that  produced  many  chicks 
throughout  the  years  presented  a problem  in  that  as  offspring  were  sent  out  to  other  zoos,  a population 
was  created  that  had  many  individuals  related  to  our  pair.  Because  of  this,  further  breeding  of  the  pair 
became  problematic  and  needed  to  be  controlled.  In  this  particular  case,  as  nests  were  built  by  the 
guans,  eggs  were  pulled  and  replaced  with  dummy  eggs.  This  helped  staff  to  manage  the  breeding, 
but  had  a negative  impact  in  that  the  male  became  quite  offended  by  the  presence  of  staff  near  his 
territory,  his  nest  or  his  mate,  and  eventually  near  his  recall  station.  A behavior  problem  was  created 
as  the  result  of  this  management  technique  but  was  later  addressed  through  training.  Inbreeding 
was  another  problem  with  a few  of  the  pigeon  species.  The  exhibit  challenges  for  staff  were  not  so 
challenging  to  prolific  birds.  Other  concerns  were  over-breeding  of  a female  (a  medical  concern), 
and  chick  abandonment  by  a female  being  constantly  courted.  All  of  these  situations  were  monitored 
and  managed  as  much  as  possible.  Training  became  a primary  way  to  step  in  and  manage  several  of 
the  situations  mentioned  above. 

Why  does  a certain  species  flourish  in  this  exhibit  when  others  don’t?  This  question  plagued  the  staff 
each  year  at  breeding  season.  The  best  conclusion  was  individual  character  and  nest  location.  A 
bird’s  ‘personality’,  whether  in  a pair  or  group  situation,  was  important  because  staff  has  noted  that 
birds  that  are  too  shy  or  too  imprinted  do  not  breed  well  in  this  exhibit.  It  was  a vast  and  extremely 
natural  exhibit,  and  birds  that  don’t  comprehend  natural  behaviors  (too  shy  or  too  imprinted)  are 
not  skilled  at  courting  or  nest  making,  which  makes  successful  breeding  nearly  impossible.  For 
example,  a slightly  imprinted  male  Sunbittem  showed  indifference  to  any  female  introduced  into 
the  exhibit.  Species  that  are  closely  related  can  be  problematic,  such  as  one  male  Bleeding  heart 
dove  that  showed  a slight  preference  to  a female  Yellow-breasted  ground  dove.  Those  situations 
were  monitored  carefully.  First  time  pairs  were  monitored  as  much  as  possible,  but  when  Crested 
oropendolas  nest  as  high  up  as  they  did,  observations  were  difficult.  In  this  particular  case,  the 
female  died  in  the  nest.  The  male  provided  staff  with  the  first  clue  when  he  stopped  visiting  the  nest, 
prompting  a tree  climbing  and  nest  removal  venture. 

All  chicks  that  hatch  are  vital  to  the  conservation  of  its  species,  whether  they  belong  to  an  SSP®, 
PMP®,  or  other  type  of  managed  program.  The  proudest  accomplishment  by  far  is  with  the  highly 
endangered  Mauritius  Pink  Pigeon  {Columba  mayeri).  For  years,  the  Rainforest  housed  non-breeding 
males  for  the  SSP®.  In  2006,  the  management  plan  allowed  an  attempted  breeding,  with  the  ultimate 
goal  to  see  if  multiple  pairs  could  eventually  be  exhibited  together.  2.1  were  housed  together  for 
four  months  before  the  first  successful  hatching  and  offspring.  This  female  chick  was  later  moved 
to  another  facility.  In  November  2007,  we  were  given  the  opportunity  to  attempt  two  pairs  of  Pink 
pigeons,  something  that  had  not  been  successful  at  other  facilities.  After  a short  acclimation  period, 
the  new  female  and  bachelor  male  paired  up  and  a few  months  later  had  a chick  - a male;  four  months 
later  they  produce  another  chick,  a female.  What  did  staff  do  to  encourage  this  success  - nothing 
beyond  a lot  of  observation.  The  exhibit  itself  provided  the  right  atmosphere  and  space  needed  to 
be  successful,  and  staff  knew  where  and  when  to  expect  chicks  once  nesting  behavior  was  observed. 
This  provided  us  with  the  upper  hand  when  it  came  to  pulling  for  banding,  health  checks,  etc.  These 
birds  proved  to  be  much  shyer  than  most  of  the  other  pigeons  on  exhibit,  and  showed  very  little 
interest  or  desire  to  participate  in  training  sessions. 

Avian  Behavior  Management 

October  2002  was  the  start  of  a training  experiment  withl.O  Black-necked  aracari  {Pteroglossus 
aracari).  The  finches  and  butterflies  that  cohabitated  the  rainforest  exhibit  with  this  aracari  were 


242  Animal  Keepers'  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5 


being  chased,  harassed,  and  on  occasion,  eaten.  The  goal  was  to  train  him  to  go  into  a crate  for 
capture.  It  was  difficult  to  recapture  fast  birds  out  of  such  a large  exhibit,  especially  one  as  intelligent 
as  this  aracari.  Staff  had  exhausted  all  resources  to  capture  or  trap  him  when  training  by  operant 
conditioning  was  suggested.  The  expectations  were  not  high,  but  training  began  anyway.  To  the 
Ij  surprise  of  the  staff,  he  was  captured  in  December  and  many  of  the  other  birds  had  begun  participating 
I in  the  training  sessions;  and  so  the  firee-flight  training  program  began  and  was  formalized  over  the 

I''  years.  What  started  as  an  experiment  and  last  ditch  effort  became  the  primary  management  tool  for 
staff  working  in  the  Rainforest. 

As  this  program  began  to  flourish,  the  main  goal  was  to  train  a few  behaviors  to  any  bird  that  wanted 
to  participate.  Participants  included  breeders,  non-breeders,  chicks,  adults,  and  singletons.  The 
time  to  train  the  birds  only  took  1 5 minutes  and  was  done  as  little  as  once  a week  or  as  often  as  five 
times  a week;  it  depended  on  the  trainer’s  schedule  for  the  day.  By  starting  at  the  entrance  to  the 
Rainforest  exhibit  and  slowly  walking  through,  the  trainer  would  stop  where  birds  were  seen  and 
\ attempted  to  train  them.  If  there  was  interest  from  the  birds,  training  occurred  and  if  there  was  not, 
i the  trainer  moved  along.  As  the  trainer’s  schedule  changed  to  care  for  different  species,  so  did  the 

ii  responsibility  of  training  the  birds.  While  the  primary  trainer  attempted  sessions  during  the  month, 
the  bird  keeper  was  encouraged  to  train  during  feeding  times  so  as  not  to  add  to  the  maintenance  of 
that  routine.  Feeding  combined  with  training  could  take  up  to  30  minutes  instead  of  10-15  minutes 
for  just  feeding.  Overall,  not  a huge  portion  of  time  to  an  already  busy  day. 


Fairy  bluebird  entering  crate. 

I ( Photo  by  Heather  Leeson ) 

< specific  location  upon  hearing  the  whistle,  it  would  not  show  up  at  a different  location  upon  hearing 
' the  shaker  can.  This  gave  shy  birds  the  chance  to  train  without  feeling  like  prey  in  a larger  group 
session.  The  crate  used  was  a Vari-kennel®  with  a wire  top  instead  of  a plastic  top.  This  was  chosen 
because  it  let  more  light  inside,  thereby  illuminating  the  entire  interior  and  allowing  birds  to  see 
I inside  from  the  front  and  top  of  the  crate.  While  inside  the  crate,  birds  were  able  to  see  the  outside 
surroundings  from  all  directions.  Feeling  comfortable  with  such  a scary  object  was  vital  to  the 
success  of  training  a bird  to  willingly  enter  and  have  the  door  closed.  The  scale  utilized  for  voluntary 
weights  was  a small  kitchen  gram  scale.  Some  birds  were  crated  then  weighed,  others  figured  out 
how  to  step  on  the  slippery  surface  to  get  weighed.  After  several  years  of  training,  staff  was  able 
to  get  monthly  weights  on  a variety  of  free-flighted  birds.  It’s  a great,  non-invasive  and  stress-free 
technique  to  enhance  husbandry  of  our  bird  collection. 

Despite  the  basic  behaviors  some  birds  displayed  behaviors  that  needed  extra  attention,  so  specific 
trained  behaviors  were  added  to  their  repertoire.  For  example,  the  male  Piping  guan’s  behavior 
became  increasingly  problematic  for  the  staff  members.  It  appeared  to  be  displaced  aggression  that 
became  self-rewarding  for  the  bird.  This  was  not  only  an  issue  for  staff,  but  a potential  issue  for  the 
public.  The  goal  was  to  teach  him  that  his  aggressive  tendencies  would  no  longer  be  tolerated.  By 
not  receiving  a reaction  from  keepers,  he  was  no  longer  being  rewarded  for  his  aggression  and  he  was 
in  a mindset  to  be  trained  to  station  and  stay.  There  were  19  pathway  benches  located  throughout 
the  exhibit,  and  he  was  rewarded  for  going  onto  any  bench  (his  station),  for  displaying  calm  body 
language  while  on  station  (no  vocalization  or  crest  raising),  and  for  staying  on  station  while  staff 


Several  basic  behaviors  were  trained 
such  as  bridge,  recall,  and  crate.  Other 
behaviors  were  added  as  needed,  such  as 
scale,  station,  and  stay.  Reinforcement 
was  always  food  from  their  daily  diet  and 
preferences  were  shown  for  peas,  com, 
grapes,  and  mealworms.  This  variety 
appealed  to  the  majority  of  the  birds, 
some  showed  a preference  for  one  food 
items,  while  others  would  work  for  two 
or  three.  The  bridge  was  a clicker.  The 
recall  sound  varied  based  on  animal  and 
locations-whistle,  shaker  can,  or  three 
rapid  clicks  of  the  clicker.  This  was  done 
to  avoid  congestion  of  animals  at  a single 
location.  If  a bird  is  trained  to  come  to  a 


Animal  Keepers'  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5  243 


moved  around  and  eventually  left  the  area.  Due  to 
years  of  receiving  high  reinforcement  for  pestering 
staff,  each  new  keeper  (regardless  of  their  section) 
was  required  to  learn  this  new  station  and  stay 
behavior  with  the  bird.  Once  the  guan  and  the  new 
keeper  roles  were  correctly  established,  there  were 
minimal  issues.  However,  during  nesting  season 
the  negative  behavior  was  likely  to  reoccur  but  was 
manageable. 

This  training  program  also  became  helpful  and 
necessary  during  emergency  situations,  such  as 
medical  concerns  or  injuries.  Birds  that  were 
crate  trained  were  easily  available  for  medical 
exam  or  treatment.  This  alleviated  stress  for  not 
only  the  bird,  but  staff  as  well.  Crate  training  and 
easy  recapture  due  to  the  successful  training  was 
the  only  way  in  which  the  staff  was  able  to  move 
animals  for  Hurricane  Rita  in  2005  and  Hurricane  Ike  in  2008. 

Offspring  of  trained  parents  were  in  a unique  position  to  learn  not  only  from  their  parents,  but  to 
learn  operant  conditioning  starting  at  only  a few  days  post-hatching.  Any  chick  seen  was  given  the 
opportunity  to  learn,  and  most  willingly  engaged  once  they  were  old  enough  to  eat  on  their  own. 
While  with  parents  they  learned  by  watching  and  became  acclimated  to  the  bridge.  Chicks  were 
taught  bridge,  recall,  and  crate.  One  of  the  hand-raised  chicks,  a Speckled  pigeon,  also  learned  step 
up  and  scale.  The  trust  that  developed  between  trainer  and  chick  as  they  grew  was  important  because 
as  they  started  to  become  more  independent,  capture  for  shipment  became  a priority.  A majority  of 
the  chicks  produced  were  trained  to  enter  a crate  voluntarily.  For  this  to  be  truly  successful  with 

chicks,  they  must  feel  comfortable  in  their 
surroundings  and  choose  to  willingly  leave 
their  parent’s  side  to  participate.  The  parents 
have  seen  this  time  and  time  again,  and  not 
only  continued  to  train,  but  continued  to 
allow  their  offspring  to  participate.  Once  the 
chicks  were  removed  from  the  exhibit  and 
moved  to  holding  for  impending  shipment, 
training  continued  so  something  familiar 
was  part  of  their  new  environment.  This  also 
benefited  the  receiving  institution  - a trusting 
relationship  with  keepers  and  three  trained 
behaviors.  By  already  being  familiar  with 
a crate,  traveling  in  one  is  not  as  stressful. 
Again,  this  only  takes  a few  minutes  to  work 
on  a few  days  a week.  The  benefits  far  outweigh  any  increased  load  to  the  standard  husbandry 
routine. 

Over  the  past  seven  years,  staff  did  not  note  any  detrimental  impact  of  training  on  the  breeding 
success  in  the  exhibit.  Ten  species  of  breeding  birds  (appx.  30  individuals)  have  participated  in  the 
training  program  since  2002.  Seven  species  of  offspring  (appx.  15  individuals)  have  been  trained. 
Overall,  what  staff  observed  was  less  stress  on  parents  and  chicks  at  the  time  of  capture  for  whatever 
reason,  trust  between  bird  (parent  or  chick)  and  trainer  never  diminished,  and  parents  continued  to 
train  willingly  year  after  year,  chick  after  chick.  It  became  an  integral  part  of  their  life  and  of  our 
bird  husbandry  routine.  A new  goal  for  staff,  once  the  collection  is  back  from  evacuation,  is  for  every 
bird  to  learn  a bridge.  This  will  pave  the  way  for  all  birds  to  learn  more  if  they  choose  and  increase 
our  husbandry  success. 

Avian  Success  and  Challenges  in  Our  Collection 

Due  to  the  large  architectural  footprint  of  the  exhibit,  the  keeper  staff  had  to  overcome  several 


0.1  Palawan  with  chick  being 


bridge  trained.  (Photo  by  Heather  Leeson) 


244  Animal  Keepers'  Forum,  VoL  37  No.  4/5 


challenges  to  ensure  not  only  the  quality  of  life  of  the  animals,  but  also  the  best  possible  guest 
experience.  These  challenges  were  met  and  often  overcome  through  ingenuity,  although  sometimes 
accepting  things  as  they  are  is  also  meeting  the  challenge. 

One  challenge  staff  faced  was  the  necessity  of  providing  feeding  stations  and  nesting  locations  that 
' were  both  accessible  for  the  birds  and  easy  for  keepers  to  observe  and  maintain.  By  strategically 
placing  feeding  stations,  staff  was  able  to  bring  some  of  the  more  colorful  birds  down  to  lower  levels 
that  would  have  normally  stayed  higher  in  the  canopy.  Again,  the  abundance  of  edible  food  naturally 
occurring  in  the  exhibit  made  this  even  more  challenging. 

Another  hurdle  to  overcome  was  the  training  program  previously  discussed.  While  not  necessarily  a 
daunting  task,  the  logistics  were.  The  nature  of  the  exhibit,  number  of  distractions  by  other  animals, 
sounds  of  the  morning  maintenance,  and  the  public  required  staff  to  keep  sessions  brief  and  behaviors 
j simple.  In  addition  to  the  distractions,  many  of  the  birds  had  short  attention  spans,  necessitating  short, 
specific  training  sessions.  Guest  interruption  could  hinder  sessions,  but  provided  an  opportunity  to 
I showcase  the  program. 

Accurate  counts  and  daily  observations  were  always  difficult,  especially  for  larger  groups  of  birds. 
One  solution  was  to  keep  a running  tally  to  track  who  had  seen  what  bird,  when,  and  where.  Using 
other  departments  (horticulture  and  education  staff  members)  also  helped  accomplish  this  goal.  If  a 
bird  was  not  observed  as  frequently  as  usual,  staff  would  start  aggressively  searching.  Often  times, 
the  absence  of  certain  birds  meant  nesting,  but  sometimes  it  meant  there  were  medical  concerns. 

The  decision  to  parent-rear  or  hand-rear  was  often  determined  by  factors  beyond  staff  control,  but 
the  exhibit  layout  and  landscaping  made  things  more  difficult  if  a chick  needed  to  be  pulled  for  any 
i reason.  As  mentioned  earlier,  nesting  sites  were  not  always  in  the  best  location  for  humans  and 
I sometimes  not  in  the  best  location  for  the  birds  either.  Although  several  birds  did  have  difficulty 
establishing  breeding  success,  others  thrived.  Mostly  a hands-off  policy  worked  and  staff  intervention 
f was  kept  to  a minimum. 

Other  challenges  faced  in  the  Rainforest  exhibit  were  not  unlike  most  other  facilities.  Often  times 
! when  there  seemed  to  be  enough  space  for  birds  to  thrive,  it  may  not  always  have  been  in  the 

[ right  kind  of  space.  Or,  where  certain  bird  species  might  have  gotten  along  in  one  situation,  once 

\ introduced  to  a new  exhibit,  new  territorial  behaviors  developed.  As  we  strived  to  maintain  a large 
I variety  of  species  and  taxa  under  one  roof,  a balance  had  to  be  achieved.  Often  times  this  balance 

would  shift  for  an  unknown  reason  resulting  in  death  or  removal  of  a species  that  didn’t  work  in  this 

environment.  Flexibility  and  observant  staff  helped  immensely.  One  example  of  this  was  the  male 
Palawan  pheasant  that  eventually  had  to  be  isolated  into  a smaller  exhibit  within  the  rainforest  due  to 
j some  aggression  issues  with  a group  of  Crested  wood  partridges 

i The  nature  of  the  Rainforest  Pyramid,  coupled  with  the  number  of  animals,  provided  a fantastic 
j opportunity  for  guests  to  view  free-flighted  birds  in  about  as  free  a state  as  one  can  find  in  a zoological 
j setting.  The  constant  enrichment  of  a large  number  of  birds,  several  levels  of  rainforest,  and  naturally 
j growing  food  sources  provided  challenges  to  husbandry.  But  these  challenges  also  led  to  many 
successes  in  husbandry  as  mentioned  throughout  this  paper.  For  the  last  1 7 years,  the  Rainforest  had 
I showcased  plant  and  animal  life  in  a unique  and  realistic  setting.  Changes  in  staff  and  collection  have 

I only  strengthened  our  ability  to  cope  with  challenges,  thereby  making  each  success,  no  matter  how 

! small,  a proud  accomplishment. 

j While  the  Rainforest  exhibit  remains  closed  to  the  public,  repairs  and  renovations  are  well  underway. 

I First  and  foremost,  plans  are  being  made  for  the  “migration”  back  to  Moody  Gardens.  The  generous 

i zoos  and  aquariums  that  offered  to  assist  us  in  our  time  of  need  are  close  by  and  bringing  animals 

back  will  be  one  less  challenge  to  face.  The  renovations  plans  include  a new  canopy  walkway 
to  provide  our  guests  with  a view  of  the  upper  portion  of  the  exhibit,  as  well  as  provide  areas  for 
keeper/guest  interaction.  New  challenges  await  the  staff  including  opportunities  to  expand  our  avian 
collection  and  husbandry  skills.  The  updates  Rainforest  Pyramid  at  Moody  Gardens  will  open  in 
Spring  2011. 


Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5  245 


Table  1 - Avian  Inventory  Moody  Gardens 


Common  Name 

Taxonomic  Name 

Exhibit 

Flighted 

American  White  Pelican 

Peiecanus  erythrorhynchos 

Flamingo  Pond 

No 

Brown  Pelican 

Pelecanus  occidentalis 

Flamingo  Pond 

No 

Scarlet  Ibis 

Eudocimus  ruber 

Rainforest 

Yes 

American  Flamingo 

Phoenicopterus  ruber 

Flamingo  Pond 

No 

Ne-ne 

Branta  sandvicensis 

Flamingo  Pond 

No 

Coscoroba  Swan 

Coscoroba  coscoroba 

Flamingo  Pond 

No 

Wood  Duck 

Aix  spoma 

Flamingo  Pond 

No 

Mandarin  Duck 

Aix  ^aiericulata 

Rainforest 

No 

Lesser  Brazilian  Teal 

Amazonetta  b.  brasiliensis 

Rainforest 

Yes 

White-cheek  Pintail 

Anas  bahamensis 

Flamingo  Pond 

No 

Cape  Teal 

Anas  capensis 

Rainforest 

No 

Baikal  Teal 

Anas  formoSQ 

Rainforest 

No 

Hottentot  Tea! 

Anas  punctata 

Rainforest 

No 

Chiloe  Wigeon 

Anas  sibilatrix 

Flamingo  Pond 

No 

Silver  Tea! 

Anas  versicolor 

Rainforest 

No 

Ringed  Teal 

Callonetta  luecophrys 

Rainforest 

No 

Red  Crested  Pochard 

Netta  ruffina 

Flamingo  Pond 

No 

South  African  Shelduck 

Tandorna  cana 

Flamingo  Pond 

No 

Radjah  Shelduck 

Tadorna  radjah 

Flamingo  Pond 

No 

Common  Shelduck 

Tadorna  tadorna 

Flamingo  Pond 

No 

Common  Piping  Guan 

Aburria  p.  cutnanensis 

Rainforest 

Yes 

Palawan  Peacock  Pheasant 

Polypiectron  emphanum 

Rainforest 

Yes 

Crested  Wood  Partridge 

Rollulus  rouloul 

Rainforest 

Yes 

Sunbittern 

Eurypyga  hetias 

Rainforest 

Yes 

Nicobar  Pigeon 

Caloenas  nicobarica 

Rainforest 

Yes 

Speckled  Pigeon 

Columba  g.  phaeonota 

Rainforest 

Yes 

Mauritius  Pink  Pigeon 

Columba  mayeri 

Rainforest 

Yes 

Pied  Imperial  Pigeon 

Ducula  bicolor 

Rainforest 

Yes 

Moluccan  Imperial  Pigeon 

Ducula  perspicillata 

Rainforest 

Yes 

Bleeding-heart  Dove 

Gallicolumba  luzonica 

Rainforest 

Yes 

Ylw.Breasted  Grnd  Dove 

Gallicolumba  tristi^maia 

Rainforest 

Yes 

Diamond  Dove 

Geopelia  cuneata 

Rainforest 

Yes 

Brozewing  Dove 

Phaps  chalcopiera 

Rainforest 

Yes 

Co  Hard  Dove 

Streptopelia  decaocto' 

Rainforest 

Yes 

White  Cockatoo 

Cacatua  alba 

Other 

No 

Sulfur  Crested  Cockatoo 

Cacatua  galerita 

Other-Education 

No 

Scarlet  chested  parrot 

Neophema  splendida 

Rainforest 

Yes 

Senega!  Parrot 

Poicephalus  senegalus 

Other-Education 

No 

Hyacinth  Macaw 

Anodorhynchus  hyacinthinus 

Rainforest-perch  birds 

No 

Hybrid  Macaw 

.Ara  species 

Rainforest-perch  birds 

No 

Blue  and  Yellow  Macaw 

Ara  ararauna 

Rainforest- 

perch/Education 

No 

Scarlet  Macaw 

Ara  macao 

Rainforest- 

perch/Education 

No 

Military  Macaw 

Ara  mil  Paris 

Rainforest-perch  birds 

No 

Monk  Parakeet 

Myiopsitta  monachus 

Rainforest 

Yes 

Orange-winged  Amazon 

Amazona  amazon ica 

Other-Education 

No 

Cuban  Amazon 

Amazona  leucocephala 

Other 

Yes 

Violet  Plantain  Eater 

Musophayta  violacea 

Rainforest 

Yes 

Blue-bellied  Roller 

Coracias  cyanoytaster 

Rainforest 

Yes 

Mexican  Emerald 

Toucanet 

Aulacorhynchus p.  prosinus 

Rainforest 

Yes 

Fairy  Bluebird 

Irena  puelia 

Rainforest 

Yes 

Saffron  Finch 

Sicalis  flaveola 

Rainforest 

Yes 

Silver-beaked  Tanager 

Ramphoceliis  carbo 

Rainforest 

Yes 

Blue-grey  Tanager 

Thraupis  episcopus 

Rainforest 

Yes 

Troupial 

Icterus  icterus 

Rainforest 

Yes 

Crested  Oropendola 

Psarocolius  decumanus 

Rainforest 

Yes 

Red-throated  parrot  finch 

Erythrura  psittacea 

Rainforest 

Yes 

Society  Finch 

Lonchura  domestica 

Rainforest 

Yes 

Zebra  Finch 

Poephila  guttata 

Rainforest 

Yes 

Orange  Bishop 

Euplectes  o.  franciscanus 

Rainforest 

Yes 

Pin-tailed  Whydah 

Vidua  macroura 

Rainforest 

Yes 

246  Animal  Keepers'  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5 


On  the  Wings  of  Cranes 

By  Lowell  M.  Schake  , 2008 

published  by  iUniverse,  Inc.  (www.iUniverse.com  ) 

ISBN-13:  978-0595484973 

Paper,  339  pages  $24.95 


Review  by  Leslie  Keys,  Principal  Keeper, 
Bird  Section,  Detroit  Zoological  Institute 
Royal  Oak,  MI 


If  John  James  Audubon  is  the  father  of  the  modem  conservation  effort,  then  Lawrence  Walkenshaw 
is  the  father  of  Crane  Biology.  As  a child,  he  was  fascinated  with  the  natural  histories  of  the  birds  in 
his  neighborhood  and  made  meticulous  notes  about  them.  As  he  grew  to  adulthood,  he  was  pressured 
into  becoming  a dentist  to  support  his  family,  but  always  his  major  interest  lay  in  the  winged  world. 
He  was  the  prime  mover  in  the  state  of  Michigan  to  study  and  protect  the  critically  endangered 
Kirtland's  warbler.  Due  to  his  observations  and  flawless  records  he  realized  long  before  others  that 
these  tiny  birds  were  in  trouble.  Always  a lover  of  sandhill  cranes,  he  became  an  expert  in  locating 
their  nests  in  Michigan  and  was  invited  to  partner  with  the  Royal  Saskatchewan  Museum  to  help 
successfiilly  locate  the  elusive  whooping  crane  nesting  area  in  Canada.  He  also  studied  sparrows, 
flycatchers  and  hundreds  of  other  species. 

With  a forward  by  Dr.  George  Archibald  of  the  International  Crane  Foundation,  this  biography  of 
Lawrence  Walkenshaw,  chronicles  the  life  of  perhaps  the  greatest  amateur  scientist  and  field  biologist 
in  recent  times.  Author  Lowell  Schake  takes  us  from  the  early  days  of  Walkenshaw  as  a child  through 
to  his  death  in  1993.  Besides  being  a history  of  Walkenshaw ’s  work  in  the  field  of  bird  research, 
Schake  shares  the  personal  life  of  this  unassuming  and  tireless  man.  He  brings  to  life  what  it  takes 
to  be  tmly  great  in  the  field  and  helps  us  realize  what  the  efforts  of  a single  dedicated  individual  can 
do  for  an  entire  field  of  study. 

This  is  a great  book  for  anyone  interested  in  field  research,  birding,  or  Mr.  Walkenshaw  himself 


Avian  Websites  to  Check  Out 

AvianWeb.com  - http://www.avianweb.com/birdspecies.htm 
The  Ornithology  Website  - http://birdwebsite.com/research/index.htm 
Birding.com  - Top  25  Birding  Sites  - http://www.birding.com/bestwebsites.asp 
Bird  Website  Links  - http://www.richard-seaman.com/Birds/links.html 
The  Cornell  Lab  of  Ornithology  - http://www.allaboutbirds.org/guide/search 
Birds  of  the  World  - http://birdix.com/main.htm 


Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5  247 


African  Penguin  (Spheniscus  demersus) 

(Photo:  Bethany  Wlaz,  Rock  Island  Keeper,  The  Maryland  Zoo  in  Baltimore) 


248  Animal  Keepers’  Forum,  Vol.  37  No.  4/5 


A AZK  Membership  Application  {Piease  Prim) 


Check  here  if  renewal  [ ] 


Name. 

Email. 


Mailing  Address 

City State/Province Zip/Postal  Code Country. 


U.S.  Members* 

□ $45.00  Professional 

I . Full-time  Keeper 

U $40.00  Affiliate 

I . Other  staff  & volunteers 

i«J  $30.00  Student 

Must  supply  copy  of  current  student 
ID  at  time  of  application 

□ $70.00  or  up  - Individuals 

Contributing/U  .S . 

□ $150.00  or  up 
Institutional/U.S. 

Organizations/ Institutions 
(requires  Board  approval) 

□ $150.00  Commercial  Member 

(i.e.  animal-related  food  & supplies) 


Canadian  Members* 

□ $45.00  Professional 

□ Full-time  Keeper 

$40.00  Affiliate 

□ Other  staff  & volunteers 

$30.00  Student 

Must  supply  copy  of  current  student 
ID  at  time  of  application 

□ $70.00  or  up  - Individuals 
Contributing/Canada 

□ $150.00  or  up 

Institutional/Canada 

Organizations!  Institutions 
( requires  Board  approval ) 

□ $45.00  Library  Only 

Available  ONLY  to  public  & 
university  libraries  (In  U.S.) 


□ $60.00  International  Members  (All  members  outside  US.  & Canada  regardless  of  category) 


Zoo  Affiliation  (if  any) 

Zoo  Address 

Title^ 

LJ  My  check  is  enclosed  (AAZK,  Inc.)  LJ  Please  charge  my  eredit  card 

Mastercard  Visa  Card# - - - 

Name  on  card Expiration  date 

Signature 

Mail  this  application  to:  AAZK  Administrative  Offices,  3601  SW  29th  St.,  Suite  133,  Topeka,  KS  66614-2054. 
Make  checks/money  orders  payable  to  AAZK,  Tnc.  Must  he  in  IJ.S.  FUNDS  ONLY.  Membership  includes  a 
subscription  to  Animal  Keepers’  Forum.  The  membership  card  is  good  for  free  or  discounted  admission  to  many 
zoos  and  aquariums  in  the  U.S.  and  Canada. 


Printed  in  U.S.  A. 
©2010  AAZK,  Inc. 


OOh- 


f-”3 

:;:o 

f-'-'tU<C 

Ll'i 
»-•• ! 

o 

r*'!! 

* 

ii”3  <ru 

:<r. 

U~J  —! 

»•■ 

o 

'At 

r--. 

:ya:  i 

i.«:E:  r-^j 

A< 

rtf  v-i 

i-.  ! O 

-O  o* 

••“'*  !L  oj 

Ar. 

A< 

A'. 

o ccgc;! 

Ac 

O 

>;■: 

At 

r-o 

At 

I---*  anr-^lf— 

A>: 

^ s,  trj 

At 

c o .'-cEr 

At 

0-4-^  >"!►-» 

At 

i-.-i-.MaizE 

At  r-l 

1 ^-1  Ijr?  CO 

At  "-- 

At  cr- 

m 


ON 


m 


o -s 
so  0 

^ m 


m 

#\ 


in 

o 

I • 

3 

N© 


"U 

® 

m 

® 

□ 

O' 

® 

DC 

® 

o 


to 

o 

LLI 

Q- 

UJ 

LU 

^5 

O 

,o 

s 

O 

rNi 

9 

•2 

&5 

o 

2 

a. 

D T^nnivepsopij 

1974  ^ 2010