Skip to main content

Full text of "The Biological bulletin"

See other formats


mi 


Volume  180 


THE 


Number  1 


BIOLOGICAL 
BULLETIN 


FEI 


FEBRUARY,  1991 


Published  by  the  Marine  Biological  Laboratory 


THE 


BIOLOGICAL  BULLETIN 


PUBLISHED  BY 
THE  MARINE  BIOLOGICAL  LABORATORY 


GEORGE  J.  AUGUSTINE,  University  of  Southern 


LOUIS  LEIBOVITZ,  Marine  Biological  Laboratory 


Editorial  Board 

RUDOLK  A.  RAI  I  .  h 
brnia 
Ki  NSAL  VAN  HOLDI 

STEVEN  VOGEL,  Du 

-MU^-U^— 

Marine  Biological  Laboratory  i 
LIBRARY 

FEB  11  1991 

diana  Llniversity 
,  Oregon  Stale  University   . 

Woods  Hole;  Mass. 

t  University 

Associate  Editors 


Peter  A.  V.  Anderson,  The  Whitney  Laboratory,  University  of  Florida 

David  Epel,  Hopkins  Marine  Station.  Stanford  University 

J.  Malcolm  Shick.  University  of  Maine.  Orono 


Editor:  MICHAEL  J.  GREENBERG,  The  Whitney  Lahoraton,'.  Llniversity  of  Florida 
Managing  Eililor  PAMELA  L.  CLAPP.  Marine  Biological  Laboratory 


FEBRUARY,  1991 


Printed  and  Issued  by 
LANCASTER  PRESS,  Inc. 

PRINCE  &  LEMON  STS. 
LANCASTER,  PA 


THE  BIOLOGICAL  BULLETIN 


THE  BIOLOGICAL  BULLETIN  is  published  six  times  a  year  by  the  Marine  Biological  Laboratory,  MBL 
Street,  Woods  Hole,  Massachusetts  02543. 

Subscriptions  and  similar  matter  should  be  addressed  to  Subscription  Manager.  THE  BIOLOGICAL  BUL- 
LETIN, Marine  Biological  Laboratory.  Woods  Hole,  Massachusetts  02543.  Single  numbers,  $25.00.  Sub- 
scription per  volume  (three  issues).  $72.50  ($145.00  per  year  for  six  issues). 

Communications  relative  to  manuscripts  should  be  sent  to  Michael  J.  Greenberg.  Editor-in-Chief,  or 
Pamela  L.  Clapp,  Managing  Editor,  at  the  Marine  Biological  Laboratory.  Woods  Hole,  Massachusetts  02543. 
Telephone:  (508)  548-3705.  ext.  428.  FAX:  508-540-6902. 


POSTMASTER:  Send  address  changes  to  THE  BIOLOGICAL  BULLETIN,  Marine  Biological  Laboratory. 

Woods  Hole,  MA  02543. 

Copyright  ici  1991.  by  the  Marine  Biological  Laboratory 

Second-class  postage  paid  at  Woods  Hole,  MA,  and  additional  mailing  offices. 

ISSN  0006-3 185 


INSTRUCTIONS  TO  AUTHORS 


The  Biological  Bulletin  accepts  outstanding  original  research 
reports  of  general  interest  to  biologists  throughout  the  world. 
Papers  are  usually  of  intermediate  length  (10-40  manuscript 
pages).  A  limited  number  of  solicited  review  papers  may  be  ac- 
cepted after  formal  review.  A  paper  will  usually  appear  within 
four  months  after  its  acceptance. 

Very  short  papers  (less  than  9  manuscript  pages  including 
tables,  figures,  and  bibliography)  will  be  published  in  a  separate 
section  entitled  "Research  Notes."  A  Research  Note  in  The  Bio- 
logical Bulletin  follows  the  format  of  similar  notes  in  Nature.  It 
should  open  with  a  summary  paragraph  of  150  to  200  words 
comprising  the  introduction  and  the  conclusions.  The  rest  of 
the  text  should  continue  on  without  subheadings,  and  there 
should  be  no  more  than  30  references.  References  should  be 
referred  to  in  the  text  by  number,  and  listed  in  the  Literature 
Cited  section  in  the  order  that  they  appear  in  the  text.  Unlike 
references  in  Nature,  references  in  the  Research  Notes  section 
should  conform  in  punctuation  and  arrangement  to  the  style  of 
recent  issues  of  The  Biological  Bulletin.  Materials  and  Methods 
should  be  incorporated  into  appropriate  figure  legends.  See  the 
article  by  Lohmann  el  al.  (October  1990.  Vol.  179:  214-218) 
for  sample  style.  A  Research  Note  will  usually  appear  within 
two  months  after  its  acceptance. 

The  Editorial  Board  requests  that  regular  manuscripts  con- 
form to  the  requirements  set  below;  those  manuscripts  that  do 
not  conform  will  be  returned  to  authors  for  correction  before 


1.  Manuscripts.  Manuscripts,  including  figures,  should 
be  submitted  in  triplicate.  (Xerox  copies  of  photographs  are  not 
acceptable  for  review  purposes.)  The  original  manuscript  must 
be  typed  in  no  smaller  than  12  pitch,  using  double  spacing  (in- 
ducting figure  legends,  footnotes,  bibliography,  etc.)  on  one  side 
of  16-  or  20-lb.  bond  paper.  8'/2  by  1 1  inches.  Please,  no  right 
justification.  Manuscripts  should  be  proofread  carefully  and  er- 
rors corrected  legibly  in  black  ink.  Pages  should  be  numbered 
consecutively.  Margins  on  all  sides  should  be  at  least  1  inch  (2.5 
cm).  Manuscripts  should  conform  to  the  Council  ot  Biology  Ed- 
itors Style  Manual.  4th  Edition  (Council  of  Biology  Editors, 
1978)  and  to  American  spelling.  Unusual  abbreviations  should 


be  kept  to  a  minimum  and  should  be  spelled  out  on  first  reference 
as  well  as  defined  in  a  footnote  on  the  title  page.  Manuscripts 
should  be  divided  into  the  following  components:  Title  page. 
Abstract  (of  no  more  than  200  words).  Introduction.  Materials 
and  Methods.  Results.  Discussion.  Acknowledgments.  Literature 
Cited.  Tables,  and  Figure  Legends.  In  addition,  authors  should 
supply  a  list  of  words  and  phrases  under  which  the  article  should 
be  indexed. 

2.  Title  page.     The  title  page  consists  of:  a  condensed  title 
or  running  head  of  no  more  than  35  letters  and  spaces,  the 
manuscript  title,  authors'  names  and  appropriate  addresses,  and 
footnotes  listing  present  addresses,  acknowledgments  or  contri- 
bution numbers,  and  explanation  of  unusual  abbreviations. 

3.  Figures.     The  dimensions  of  the  printed  page,  7  by  9 
inches,  should  be  kept  in  mind  in  preparing  figures  for  publi- 
cation. We  recommend  that  figures  be  about  1 '/:  times  the  linear 
dimensions  of  the  final  printing  desired,  and  that  the  ratio  of 
the  largest  to  the  smallest  letter  or  number  and  of  the  thickest 
to  the  thinnest  line  not  exceed  1:1.5.  Explanatory  matter  gen- 
erally should  be  included  in  legends,  although  axes  should  always 
be  identified  on  the  illustration  itself.  Figures  should  be  prepared 
for  reproduction  as  either  line  cuts  or  halftones.  Figures  to  be 
reproduced  as  line  cuts  should  be  unmounted  glossy  photo- 
graphic reproductions  or  drawn  in  black  ink  on  white  paper, 
good-quality  tracing  cloth  or  plastic,  or  blue-lined  coordinate 
paper.  Those  to  be  reproduced  as  halftones  should  be  mounted 
on  board,  with  both  designating  numbers  or  letters  and  scale 
bars  affixed  directly  to  the  figures.  All  figures  should  be  numbered 
in  consecutive  order,  with  no  distinction  between  text  and  plate 
figures.  The  author's  name  and  an  arrow  indicating  orientation 
should  appear  on  the  reverse  side  of  all  figures. 

4.  Tables,  footnotes,  figure  legends,  etc.     Authors  should 
follow  the  style  in  a  recent  issue  of  The  Biological  Bulletin  in 
preparing  table  headings,  figure  legends,  and  the  like.  Because 
of  the  high  cost  of  setting  tabular  material  in  type,  authors  are 
asked  to  limit  such  material  as  much  as  possible.  Tables,  with 
their  headings  and  footnotes,  should  be  typed  on  separate  sheets, 
numbered  with  consecutive  Roman  numerals,  and  placed  after 


the  Literature  Cited.  Figure  legends  should  contain  enough  in- 
formation to  make  the  figure  intelligible  separate  from  the  text. 
Legends  should  be  typed  double  spaced,  with  consecutive  Arabic 
numbers,  on  a  separate  sheet  at  the  end  of  the  paper.  Footnotes 
should  be  limited  to  authors'  current  addresses,  acknowledg- 
ments or  contribution  numbers,  and  explanation  of  unusual 
abbreviations.  All  such  footnotes  should  appear  on  the  title  page. 
Footnotes  are  not  normally  permitted  in  the  body  of  the  text. 

5.  Literature  cited.  In  the  text,  literature  should  be  cited 
by  the  Harvard  system,  with  papers  by  more  than  two  authors 
cited  as  Jones  el  ai.  1980.  Personal  communications  and  ma- 
terial in  preparation  or  in  press  should  be  cited  in  the  text  only, 
with  author's  initials  and  institutions,  unless  the  material  has 
been  formally  accepted  and  a  volume  number  can  be  supplied. 
The  list  of  references  following  the  text  should  be  headed  Lit- 
erature Cited,  and  must  be  typed  double  spaced  on  separate 
pages,  conforming  in  punctuation  and  arrangement  to  the  style 
of  recent  issues  of  The  Biological  Bulletin.  Citations  should  in- 
clude complete  titles  and  inclusive  pagination.  Journal  abbre- 
viations should  normally  follow  those  of  the  U.  S.  A.  Standards 
Institute  (USASI),  as  adopted  by  BIOLOGICAL  ABSTRACTS  and 
CHEMICAL  ABSTRACTS,  with  the  minor  differences  set  out  below. 
The  most  generally  useful  list  of  biological  journal  titles  is  that 
published  each  year  by  BIOLOGICAL  ABSTRACTS  (BIOSIS  List  of 
Serials;  the  most  recent  issue).  Foreign  authors,  and  others  who 
are  accustomed  to  using  THE  WORLD  LIST  OF  SCIENTIFIC  PE- 
RIODICALS, may  find  a  booklet  published  by  the  Biological 
Council  of  the  U.K.  (obtainable  from  the  Institute  of  Biology, 
41  Queen's  Gate.  London.  S.W.7.  England.  U.K.)  useful,  since 
it  sets  out  the  WORLD  LIST  abbreviations  for  most  biological 
journals  with  notes  of  the  USASI  abbreviations  where  these  differ. 
CHEMICAL  ABSTRACTS  publishes  quarterly  supplements  of  ad- 
ditional abbreviations.  The  following  points  of  reference  style 
for  THE  BIOLOGICAL  BULLETIN  differ  from  USASI  (or  modified 
WORLD  LIST)  usage: 


A.  Journal  abbreviations,  and  book  titles,  all  underlined 
(for  italics) 

B.  All  components  of  abbreviations  with  initial  capitals 
(not  as  European  usage  in  WORLD  LIST  e.g.  J  Cell  (.  'oinp.  Phy>- 
/<>/.  NOT/  cell.  comp.  Physiol.) 

C.  All  abbreviated  components  must  be  followed  by  a  pe- 
riod, whole  word  components  must  not  (i.e.  J  Cancer  Rc\  ) 

D.  Space  between  all  components  (e.g.  J    Cell.  Co/up. 
Physiol.,  not  J.CM.Comp.Phyaiol.) 

E.  Unusual  words  in  journal  titles  should  be  spelled  out 
in  full,  rather  than  employing  new  abbreviations  invented  by 
the  author.  For  example,  use  Rii  I  'isindafielags  hleiulinga  with- 
out abbreviation. 

F.  All  single  word  journal  titles  in  full  (e .g.  I  'eliger.  Ecology, 
Brain). 

C.  The  order  of  abbreviated  components  should  be  the 
same  as  the  word  order  of  the  complete  title  (;.i-.  Proc.  and  Trans. 
placed  where  they  appear,  not  transposed  as  in  some  BIOLOGICAL 
ABSTRACTS  listings). 

H.  A  few  well-known  international  journals  in  their  pre- 
ferred forms  rather  than  WORLD  LIST  or  USASI  usage  (e.g.  Na- 
ture, Science.  Evolution  NOT  Nature.  Lund.,  Science.  N.Y.; 
Evolution.  Lancaster.  Pa.) 

6.  Reprints,  page  proofs,  and  charges.  Authors  receive  their 
first  100  reprints  (without  covers)  free  of  charge.  Additional  re- 
prints may  be  ordered  at  time  of  publication  and  normally  will 
be  delivered  about  two  to  three  months  after  the  issue  date. 
Authors  (or  delegates  for  foreign  authors)  will  receive  page  proofs 
of  articles  shortly  before  publication.  They  will  be  charged  the 
current  cost  of  printers'  time  for  corrections  to  these  (other  than 
corrections  of  printers'  or  editors'  errors).  Other  than  these 
charges  for  authors'  alterations.  The  Biological  Bulletin  does  not 
have  page  charges. 


Reference:  Bid.  Hull  180:  1-11.  (l-'ehruary,  1991) 


Chemical  Mediation  of  Larval  Release  Behaviors 
in  the  Crab  Neopanope  sayi 


M.  C.  DE  VRIES,  D.  RITTSCHOF.  AND  R.  B.  FORWARD  JR. 

Duke  University  Marine  Laboratory,  Beaufort,  North  Carolina  28516  and  Duke  University 
Zoologv  Department.  Durham.  North  Carolina  27706 


Abstract.  Control  of  egg  hatching  was  investigated  in 
ovigerous  females  of  the  crab  Neopanope  sayi.  Larval  re- 
lease is  a  brief  event,  generally  lasting  less  than  15  min, 
during  which  females  perform  stereotypic  behaviors  in- 
volving vigorous  abdomen  pumping.  Substances  released 
by  hatching  eggs  (pumping  factors)  of  TV.  sayi.  Rhithro- 
panopeus  harrisii,  and  Uea  pugilator,  but  not  Sesarma 
cinereiim,  evoked  these  stereotypic  behaviors  (pumping 
response)  in  ovigerous  N.  sayi.  Spontaneous  pumping  and 
responsiveness  to  pumping  factors  varied  with  the  age  of 
the  embryos.  These  results  indicate  that  the  eggs  release 
pheromones  around  the  time  of  hatching,  which  supports 
the  general  model  for  egg-hatching  control  described  for 
R.  harrisii  (Forward  and  Lohmann,  1983).  The  chemistry 
of  N.  sayi  pumping  factors  was  investigated,  and  the 
pumping  response  was  used  as  a  bioassay  in  this  study. 
Pumping  factors  adsorbed  to  Amberlite  XAD-7  resin  and 
could  be  eluted  from  it  with  methanol.  Size  fractionation 
by  cascade  pressure  dialysis  showed  that  the  active  mol- 
ecules were  <1000  daltons.  Acid  hydrolysis  followed  by 
reverse-phase  HPLC  amino  acid  analysis  showed  that  the 
biologically  active  fraction  contained  peptides.  Cysteine, 
glycine,  methionine,  and  isoleucine  were  the  four  most 
common  amino  acids  in  these  peptides.  The  responsive- 
ness of  N.  sayi  to  hatch  water  from  R  harrisii.  the  general 
similarity  of  adsorptive  characteristics  of  hatch  waters 
from  the  two  species  toward  XAD-7  resin,  and  the  amino 
acid  compositional  analysis  suggest  that  the  pumping  fac- 
tors from  both  species  are  similar.  This  supports  the  hy- 
pothesis that  N.  sayi  pumping  factors  are  also  small  pep- 
tides, as  was  suggested  for  those  of  R.  harrisii  (Rittschof 
etal..  1985,  1989). 

Introduction 

Rhythms  in  larval  release  corresponding  to  lunar,  diel, 
and  tidal  cycles  have  been  observed  for  numerous  species 

Received  15  December  1989;  accepted  28  November  1990. 


of  Brachyura  (see  DeCoursey,  1983;  Forward,  1987,  for 
reviews).  In  species  showing  rhythms,  egg  hatching  gen- 
erally occurs  during  the  dark  phase  of  the  diel  cycle,  and 
often  near  the  high  tide  of  a  tidal  cycle  (Saigusa  and  Hi- 
daka,  1978;  Saigusa,  1981,  1982;  Forward  et  ai.  1982; 
Wolcott  and  Wolcott,  1982;  Christy,  1986;  Salmon  et  ai, 
1986;  De  Vries  and  Forward,  1989).  For  most  warm-water 
species,  such  as  the  xanthid  Neopanope  sayi,  larval  release 
is  a  brief  event,  usually  lasting  less  than  15  min  for  an 
individual  (DeCoursey,  1979;  Forward  el  ai.  1982;  De 
Vries,  1990).  During  this  time,  in  general,  all  of  a  female's 
eggs  hatch  while  the  female  vigorously  pumps  her  abdo- 
men. Occasionally,  larvae  are  released  in  more  than  one 
short  burst  at  the  time  of  consecutive  tidal  phases  or  nights 
(Forward  el  ai.  1982;  Christy,  1986;  De  Vries  and  For- 
ward. 1989). 

The  control  of  egg-hatching  time  in  decapods  seems  to 
vary  with  species.  Hatching  time  has  been  reported  to  be 
controlled  by  the  female  (Branford,  1978;  DeCoursey, 
1979)  and  alternatively  by  the  developing  embryos  (Pan- 
dian,  1970;  Ennis,  1973;  Forward  and  Lohmann,  1983) 
in  crabs  and  lobsters.  The  site  of  egg-hatching  control  may 
also  be  related  to  adult  habitat  (De  Vries  and  Forward, 
1 99 1  a).  Control  of  egg-hatching  time  has  been  well  studied 
only  in  the  subtidal  xanthid  crab  Rhithropanopeus  harrisii 
(Forward  and  Lohmann,  1983;  Rittschof  et  ai.  1985; 
Forward  et  ai,  1987;  Rittschof  etal..  1989).  In  this  species, 
the  developing  embryos  control  the  exact  timing,  while 
the  female  controls  the  synchrony  of  hatching.  Substances 
associated  with  hatching  eggs  are  released  near  the  time 
of  hatching  and  induce  the  ovigerous  female  to  perform 
stereotyped  larval  release  behaviors  that  synchronize 
hatching.  These  pheromones  are  collectively  called 
"pumping  factors"  and  are  a  heterogeneous  group  of  pep- 
tides mostly  of  <500  daltons. 

The  present  study  was  performed  to  investigate  the 
generality  of  the  model  for  hatching-time  control  de- 


M.  C.  DE  VRIES  ET  AL. 


scribed  for  R.  harrisii  (Forward  and  Lohmann,  1983).  In 
particular,  we  aimed  to  determine  whether  active  sub- 
stances from  another  xanthid,  Neopanope  sayi,  are  similar 
to  active  components  from  R.  harrisii.  If  active  molecules 
from  N.  sayi  are  similar  to  those  from  R  harrisii,  they 
should  cross  react  biologically,  and  produce  similar  results 
upon  chemical  purification  and  analysis,  as  done  for 
R.  harrisii  pumping  (actors  (Rittschof  ct  a/..  1985). 

Neopanope  sayi  is  a  subtidal  xanthid  crab,  occurring 
in  coastal  and  estuarine  areas,  from  the  low  littoral  to  the 
sublittoral  zones  (Williams,  1984).  Experiments  were  de- 
signed to  determine  whether  hatching  eggs  of  A',  sayi  and 
other  crab  species  produce  substances  that  stimulate  ovi- 
gerous  N.  sayi  to  perform  larval  release  behaviors.  Pump- 
ing factors  were  indicated,  and  some  of  their  chemical 
characteristics  were  investigated,  the  crabs'  stereotyped 
larval  release  behavior  serving  as  an  assay  for  biological 
activity.  Our  results  suggest  that  pumping  factors  from  N. 
sayi  are  similar  in  composition,  but  not  identical,  to  those 
from  R.  harrisii. 

Materials  and  Methods 

General  collection  and  maintenance  of  animals 

Ovigerous  females  of  Neopanope  sayi  (Smith)  were  col- 
lected from  among  the  subtidal  hard  substrate  community 
near  the  Duke  University  Marine  Laboratory  in  Beaufort, 
North  Carolina.  Crabs  were  brought  into  the  laboratory 
and  placed  into  individually  numbered  culture  bowls  (di- 
ameter, 10.4  cm)  containing  approximately  160  ml  of  5 
^m  filtered  ambient  salinity  seawater  (approximately  32- 
35%o).  Crabs  were  located  in  a  controlled-environment 
room  (27°C  ±  1°C),  under  a  14  h  light:  10  h  dark  cycle, 
with  lights-out  at  2000  h.  This  LD  cycle  corresponded  to 
the  cycle  in  the  field  at  the  time  of  collection. 

The  water  in  each  crab's  bowl  was  changed  daily  be- 
tween 0900  and  1200  h.  At  this  time,  the  presence  of 
larvae  in  the  bowls  was  noted  and  the  date  of  larval  release 
recorded  for  each  crab.  Experiments  were  performed  on 
ovigerous  females  and  the  data  were  examined  in  relation 
to  the  age  of  the  embryo.  Embryonic  age  at  the  time  of 
experimentation  (expressed  as  days  until  hatching)  was 
determined  by  counting  backwards  from  the  subsequent 
time  of  hatching.  For  individuals  that  released  larvae  in 
more  than  one  burst,  the  release  date  was  considered  to 
be  on  the  day  that  the  first  group  of  eggs  hatched.  Crabs 
were  not  fed  while  in  the  laboratory. 

Spontaneous  levels  of  pumping 

Until  they  released  their  larvae,  the  crabs  were  placed 
once  each  day  for  2  min  into  filtered  (0.45  /urn)  seawater, 
and  the  number  of  spontaneous  pumps  counted.  The 
spontaneous  pumping  activities  of  62  N.  savi  carrying 
embryos  of  various  developmental  stages  were  recorded 


in  this  way.  These  data  were  collected  to  determine 
whether  the  percentage  of  crabs  that  pumped  sponta- 
neously, and  the  absolute  frequency  of  spontaneous 
pumping  varied  with  embryo  age. 

Because  crabs  were  brought  into  the  laboratory  carrying 
embryos  of  all  ages,  and  because  their  pumping  activities 
were  measured  repeatedly,  the  effects  on  spontaneous 
pumping  activity  of  length  of  time  in  the  laboratory  and 
of  embryo  age  might  be  confounded.  To  separate  the  ef- 
fects of  the  two  variables,  plots  of  frequency  of  sponta- 
neous abdomen  pumping  versus  length  of  time  in  the 
laboratory  were  made  for  three  groups  of  crabs  (i.e..  those 
carrying  embryos  that  would  hatch  in  0-1,  2-3,  and  4-5 
days,  respectively).  These  plots  showed  no  relationship 
between  pumping  and  time  in  the  laboratory  (De  Vries, 
unpub.  data).  Thus,  when  embryo  age  was  held  constant, 
the  frequency  of  spontaneous  abdomen  pumping  ap- 
peared to  be  independent  of  time  in  the  laboratory.  Plots 
were  not  made  for  crabs  carrying  embryos  in  earlier  stages 
because  of  the  small  numbers  and  extremely  low  pumping 
rate  of  such  crabs.  N.  sayi  release  their  larvae  within  at 
most  10  days  of  egg  deposition  at  the  laboratory  mainte- 
nance temperature  of  27°C. 

Biological  assays 

An  abdomen-pumping  bioassay  was  used  to  detect 
chemicals  that  stimulated  larval  release  behaviors  in  ovi- 
gerous crabs.  The  assay,  a  modification  of  that  described 
in  Forward  and  Lohmann  (1983),  proceeded  as  follows. 
A  crab  was  placed  in  a  bowl  (diameter,  7.9  cm)  containing 
80  ml  of  filtered,  (0.45  ^m)  ambient  salinity  seawater,  and 
the  frequency  of  abdomen  pumps  was  counted  for  2  min. 
The  crab  was  then  transferred  to  a  second  bowl  containing 
80  ml  of  test  solution,  and  the  count  was  repeated.  If  a 
crab  pumped  at  least  five  more  times  in  the  second  bowl 
than  in  the  first,  this  was  counted  as  a  positive  signal  or 
a  "response"  to  that  test  solution.  For  any  given  test  so- 
lution, 20-60  (but  usually  30)  animals  were  assayed.  The 
percentage  of  crabs  tested  that  responded  to  each  test  so- 
lution was  defined  as  the  %  response,  and  is  considered  a 
measure  of  the  biological  activity  of  that  solution.  Most 
test  solutions  were  derived  from  water  in  which  crab  larvae 
were  released  by  females.  We  used  the  number  of  larvae 
released  per  ml  of  water  as  an  indication  of  the  concen- 
tration of  active  substances  in  that  solution.  The  results 
of  pumping-response  assays  are  shown  as  dose-response 
curves  (i.e.,  larvae/ml  vs.  %  response),  such  as  that  in  Fig- 
ure 2. 

The  two-bowl  protocol,  described  above,  was  used  to 
allow  for  variability  in  spontaneous  pumping  activity 
among  individuals,  as  well  as  for  changes  in  this  parameter 
within  an  individual  that  might  occur  between  1000  and 
1700  h,  the  interval  during  which  these  assays  were  per- 
formed. An  assay  was  also  performed  in  which  both  bowls 


CONTROL  OF  CRAB  LARVAL  RELEASE 


contained  filtered  seawater — a  control  of  the  effects  of  the 
experimental  procedure  upon  spontaneous  pumping  rates. 
Although  pumps  were  usually  vigorous,  they  were  some- 
times subtle,  and  could  be  unseen  if  crabs  suddenly  moved 
their  abdomens  out  of  view.  The  criterion  of  a  five  pump 
difference  to  define  a  positive  response  was  therefore  used 
to  preclude  potential  observational  errors  that  could  occur 
with  differences  of  less  than  five  pumps.  A  simple  pro- 
portional increase  in  pumping  between  two  bowls  to  de- 
fine a  response  was  inappropriate,  because  many  crabs 
pumped  0  times  in  the  first  bowl. 

Water  in  the  control  and  test  bowls  was  replaced  after 
every  10  crabs  to  ensure  a  minimal  change  in  water  com- 
position between  crabs  (Forward  et  ai,  1987).  Individuals 
were  assayed  in  each  concentration  of  a  test  solution  only 
once,  and  were  not  retested  with  another  concentration 
within  30  min.  Individuals  were  generally  tested  3-5 
times/day.  Crabs  were  returned  to  their  home  bowls  con- 
taining filtered  seawater  between  tests.  Substances  were 
tested  from  the  lowest  to  the  highest  concentrations  to 
reduce  adaptation.  Significant  differences  between  test  and 
control  response  levels  were  established  by  the  use  of  a 
Z-statistic  for  testing  differences  between  two  proportions 
at  a  =  0.05  (Walpole,  1974). 

Preparation  oj  hatch  water 

We  collected  water  into  which  N.  sari  had  released  lar- 
vae and  determined  whether  it  would  stimulate  larval  re- 
lease behaviors  (i.e.,  abdominal  pumping)  in  ovigerous 
crabs.  About  1-2  h  before  the  predicted  time  of  larval 
release  (generally  evening  high  tide  for  N.  sayi:  De  Vries 
and  Forward,  1989),  ovigerous  crabs  were  placed  into  in- 
dividual culture  bowls.  The  bowls  were  10.4  or  7.9  cm  in 
diameter  (depending  upon  crab  size)  and  contained  100 
or  50  ml  (respectively)  of  0.45  ^m  filtered  ambient  salinity 
seawater.  Just  after  a  female  had  released  her  larvae,  she 
was  removed  from  the  bowl,  and  the  water  was  passed 
through  100  nm  plankton  netting  to  remove  the  larvae. 
This  filtered  hatch  water  was  kept  on  ice  only  briefly  and 
was  then  frozen  (-20°C)  for  later  use.  The  liter  of  pump- 
ing factors  in  a  hatch  water  sample  was  estimated  by 
counting  subsamples  of  the  larvae  contributing  to  it,  and 
is  expressed  as  larvae/ml.  Hatch  water  was  collected  from 
three  additional  crab  species,  Sesarma  cinerewn  (Grap- 
sidae),  Uca  pugilator  (Ocypodidae),  and  Rhithropanopeus 
harrisii  (Xanthidae),  as  described  above,  except  that  R. 
harrisii  hatch  water  was  collected  in  10%o  water,  as  ne- 
cessitated by  their  upper  estuarine  habitat.  For  testing  in 
biological  assays  with  N.  sayi,  the  hatch  water  from  R. 
harrisiivtas  raised  to  ambient  salinity  with  Instant  Ocean. 

The  response  to  hatch  water  from  A',  sayi  was  assayed 
with  crabs  carrying  embryos  of  different  ages  to  determine 
whether  sensitivity  changed  with  the  stage  of  embryonic 
development.  Crabs  with  early  (>5  days  until  hatching) 


embryos  and  crabs  with  late-stage  (<3  days  until  hatching) 
embryos  were  tested,  and  control  levels  (i.e.,  percent  re- 
sponse) were  established  for  them.  For  all  other  assays, 
however,  only  crabs  with  late-stage  embryos  were  used. 

Isolation  and  purification  of  pumping  factors 

Adsorption  chromatography.  The  molecular  character- 
istics of  N.  sayi  pumping  factors  were  determined  by  a 
modification  of  the  adsorption  chromatography  and  size 
fractionation  procedures  of  Rittschof  et  ai  (1985),  with 
the  pumping  bioassay  being  used  to  monitor  the  process. 
The  pumping  factors  from  hatch  water  were  first  concen- 
trated on  Amberlite  XAD-7  resin.  The  column  of  resin 
(24  cm  X  1  cm  bed  volume)  was  stored  in  100%  HPLC- 
grade  methanol  (Fisher  Chemical  Co.),  and  immediately 
before  being  loaded  with  pumping  factors  was  rinsed  with 
hexane,  then  back  flushed  and  rinsed  with  at  least  200  ml 
of  deionized  water.  Loading  was  done  by  gravity-feed  at 
approximately  16  ml/min.  The  passage  of  hatch  water 
through  the  column  was  stopped  before  the  resin  was  ex- 
posed. Methanol  was  carefully  overlaid  upon  the  hatch 
water.  At  the  first  signs  of  methanol  breakthrough  in  the 
eluate  (decrease  in  drop  size  and  effervescence),  the  next 
1 3  ml  of  solution  was  collected.  The  methanol  in  this 
sample  was  then  evaporated  under  a  stream  of  N2  until 
approximately  1-2  ml  of  solution  remained.  This  con- 
centrated pumping  factor  was  stored  at  — 20°C  until  it 
was  bioassayed  or  size  fractionated. 

For  bioassays,  concentrated  pumping  factor  was  diluted 
with  filtered  (0.45  urn)  seawater  to  the  desired  test  con- 
centration. The  calculation  of  larval  concentrations  in  the 
test  solutions  was  based  upon  that  estimated  in  the  original 
hatch  water  samples,  assuming  100%  recovery  of  pumping 
factors  from  the  resin. 

Cascade  pressure  dialysis.  Hatch  water  from  N.  sayi 
that  had  been  concentrated  on  XAD-7  resin  was  brought 
to  a  volume  of  about  100  ml  with  deionized  water.  This 
solution  was  subjected  to  cascade  pressure  dialysis  (4°C, 
40  psi);  Amicon  YM 10  and  YM2  Diaflo  membranes  with 
nominal  cutoffs  at  10  and  1  kDa,  respectively,  were  used. 
The  membranes  were  stored  and  rinsed  according  to  the 
manufacturer's  instructions.  Two  additional  rinses  with 
50  ml  deionized  water  were  carried  out  under  pressure  to 
insure  that  all  preservatives  were  washed  from  the  mem- 
branes. A  sample  was  first  passed  through  the  10  kDa 
cutoff  membrane.  Part  of  this  filtrate  was  bioassayed,  and 
the  remainder  was  fractionated  with  the  1  kDa  cutoff 
membrane.  When  about  10  ml  of  sample  remained  above 
each  membrane,  three  successive  40  ml  rinses  with  dis- 
tilled water  were  done.  This  procedure  effectively  elimi- 
nated small  molecules  (<10  or  <1  kDa)  in  the  original 
solution  that  might  have  been  passively  retained  above 
the  membranes.  Rinse  water  passing  through  the  mem- 
branes was  discarded.  Those  solutions  that  passed  through 


M.  C.  DE  VRIES  /:/     I/ 


the  membranes,  as  well  as  those  that  were  retained,  were 
kept  on  ice  and  bioassayed  immediately. 

For  bioassays,  size  fractionated  pumping  factor  was  di- 
luted with  filtered  (0.45  nm)  seawater  to  the  desired  test 
concentration.  Larval  concentrations  in  the  test  solutions 
were  calculated  based  on  those  estimated  in  the  original 
hatch  water  sample,  assuming  100%  recovery  of  pumping 
factors  from  size  fractionation  procedures. 

Control  solutions 

To  be  certain  that  substances  with  biological  activity 
were  directly  related  to  the  hatching  process,  two  control 
solutions  were  subjected  to  the  hatch  water  purification 
procedure  described  above.  One  solution  was  0.45  ^m 
filtered  seawater.  The  other  was  seawater  in  which  ovi- 
gerous  crabs  had  been  incubated  under  conditions  similar 
to  those  used  for  crabs  releasing  larvae  (ovigerous  crab 
essence).  The  filtered  seawater  was  processed  to  ensure 
that  no  stimulatory  effects  were  produced  by  the  adsorp- 
tion chromatography  or  cascade  pressure  dialysis.  A  vol- 
ume of  seawater  equal  to  the  average  volume  of  hatch 
water  processed  in  each  batch  was  used.  The  number  of 
larval  equivalents  in  the  filtered  seawater  control  was  based 
on  the  average  concentration  of  all  N.  sayi  hatch  water 
processed  during  the  present  experiments. 

Ovigerous  crab  essence  was  included  as  a  control  to 
ensure  that  substances  associated  with  hatching  eggs,  and 
not  substances  secreted  by  ovigerous  crabs  or  their  em- 
bryos at  other  times,  were  responsible  for  the  observed 
pumping  activity.  Ovigerous  crab  essence  was  prepared 
by  placing  20  ovigerous  N.  sayi  with  embryos  of  various 
developmental  stages  into  2  1  of  0.45  /urn  filtered  seawater. 
After  2  h,  the  crabs  were  removed  and  the  water  treated 
as  described  above  for  the  filtered  seawater  control.  Each 
crab  was  assumed  to  carry  2500  eggs  (based  on  unpub- 
lished estimates  of  egg-mass  sizes  for  N.  sayi),  from  which 
the  larval  concentrations  of  this  control  were  calculated. 
Aliquots  of  the  seawater  and  ovigerous  crab  essence  con- 
trols were  diluted  such  that  they  contained  concentrations 
equivalent  to  20  and  50  larvae/ml  and  were  assayed:  these 
concentrations  of  crude  and  size-fractionated  hatch  water 
produced  strong  pumping  responses. 

To  be  certain  that  larger  active  molecules  were  not  de- 
natured on  the  column  matrix  upon  elution,  hatch  water 
not  passed  through  the  column  was  filtered  through  a  10 
kDa  membrane.  Dilution  series  of  the  <  10  kDa  and  >  10 
kDa  fractions  were  assayed. 

Amino  acid  analysis 

The  amino  acid  composition  of  N.  sari  pumping  factors 
was  analyzed  by  Dr.  Dano  Fiorio  at  Florida  State  Uni- 
versity. A  <  1  kDa  sample  from  the  release  of  approxi- 
mately 19,000  larvae  was  analyzed.  Reverse-phase  high- 
performance  liquid  chromatography  (HPLC)  and  pre- 


column  derivatization  with  phenylisothiocyanate  were 
performed  using  a  modification  of  the  method  in  Hen- 
rickson  and  Meredith  (1984).  Unhydrolyzed  and  hydro- 
lyzed  (in  6  N  HC1  for  24  h  at  1 10°C)  samples  were  ana- 
lyzed to  determine  the  initial  composition  of  free  amino 
acids  and  the  composition  of  the  peptides  (<1  kDa),  re- 
spectively. Phenylthiocarbomyl  derivatives  were  separated 
on  an  octadecasilyl  reverse-phase  column  and  detected 
spectrophotometrically  at  254  nm.  Identification  and 
quantification  of  amino  acids  was  by  comparison  of  de- 
rivatized  standard  amino  acids  with  those  in  the  samples. 

Amino  acid  experiment* 

Results  of  the  above  compositional  analysis  provided 
the  basis  for  testing  the  biological  activity  of  mixtures  of 
pure  amino  acids  (Sigma  Chemicals).  Pumping  assays 
were  performed  using  a  mixture  of  the  four  most  abundant 
amino  acids  in  hydrolyzed  pumping  factor,  L-cysteine, 
glycine.  L-isoleucine,  and  L-methionine.  These  amino 
acids  were  combined  in  equimolar  amounts  (as  in  hydro- 
lyzed factor)  and  tested  at  concentrations  bracketing  those 
for  which  hatch  water  was  active.  In  addition,  pumping 
assays  were  performed  using  a  combination  of  glycine 
and  arginine,  the  most  abundant  amino  acids  in  Rhith- 
ropanopeux  harrisii  pumping  factor,  in  proportions  and 
concentrations  bracketing  their  level  in  hydrolyzed 
pumping  factor  from  this  species  (Rittschof  et  at.,  1985). 

Results 

Spontaneous  pumping  rates 

Frequency  of  spontaneous  abdomen  pumping  generally 
increased  with  the  age  of  the  embryos  (Fig.  1A).  For  crabs 
with  embryos  of  all  ages,  at  least  25%  did  not  pump  at 
all.  However,  the  percentage  of  crabs  which  pumped  in- 
creased sharply  with  increasing  embryo  age  (Fig.  IB). 

Response  to  hatch  water 

The  percent  pumping  response  of  ovigerous  N.  sayi 
individuals  increased  when  the  animals  were  exposed  to 
hatch  water  (Fig.  2A).  Responsiveness  varied  with  con- 
centration for  crabs  with  both  early-  and  late-stage  em- 
bryos. At  concentrations  lower  than  2.5  larvae/ml  for 
crabs  with  late  embryos,  and  lower  than  5.0  larvae/ml  for 
crabs  with  early  embryos,  the  percentages  of  crabs  re- 
sponding were  not  significantly  different  from  controls. 
At  these  concentrations  and  higher,  however,  the  per- 
centages of  crabs  responding  were  significantly  greater 
than  controls. 

For  each  concentration  tested,  the  percentages  of  crabs 
with  early  embryos  that  responded  were  consistently  lower 
than  those  of  crabs  with  late  embryos.  This  reflects,  in 
part,  the  greater  inclination  of  crabs  with  older  embryos 
to  spontaneously  pump  their  abdomens  (as  evidenced  by 


CONTROL  OF  CRAB  LARVAL  RELEASE 


the  increased  control  levels),  but  probably  also  indicates 
that  crabs  with  older  embryos  are  more  sensitive  or  re- 
sponsive to  pumping  factors.  The  latter  is  evidenced  by 
the  higher  concentration  of  hatch  water  necessary  to  elicit 
a  significant  percent  response  for  crabs  with  early  embryos 
compared  to  those  with  late  embryos.  Because  crabs  with 
late  stage  embryos  were  more  responsive  to  pumping  fac- 
tors, they  were  used  in  all  subsequent  experiments. 

N.  sayi  with  late  embryos  also  had  significantly  higher 
percent  pumping  responses  upon  exposure  to  hatch  water 
from  Rhithropanopeus  harrixii  and  Uca  pugilator  at  con- 
centrations >  20  larvae/ml  (Fig.  3).  Exposure  of  N.  sayi 
to  hatch  water  from  Sesarnw  cincrcnm  at  concentrations 
from  1  to  60  larvae/ml,  however,  produced  levels  of  re- 
sponse not  significantly  different  from  the  control  (Fig. 
3).  These  results  indicate  some,  but  not  complete,  cross- 
reactivity  of  hatch  waters  among  species,  and  suggest  that 
active  substances  from  some  species  are  similar  in  com- 
position. 

Adsorption  chromatography  and 
cascade  pressure  dialysis 

Hatch  water  was  fractionated  into  substances  with  and 
without  affinity  for  Amberlite  XAD-7  resin,  and  the  frac- 
tions were  bioassayed  with  crabs  bearing  late-stage  em- 
bryos. A  concentration  of  10  larvae/ml  was  tested  because 
it  produced  maximum  response  (Fig.  2A).  When  exposed 
to  untreated  hatch  water,  69%  of  the  crabs  responded  (Ta- 
ble I),  which  was  significantly  greater  than  the  control 
level  (23%;  n  =  158).  After  passage  of  hatch  water  through 
the  resin  however,  activity  was  lost.  Only  33%  of  the  crabs 
responded,  which  was  not  significantly  different  from  the 
control  level. 

To  recover  adsorbed  activity,  the  resin  was  eluted  with 
methanol,  which  removes  lipophilic  and  proteinaceous 
substances.  Bioassays  showed  a  modest  increase  in  re- 
sponse when  tested  with  the  methanol  eluate,  but  at  con- 
centrations up  to  20  larvae/ml,  these  responses  were  not 
significantly  different  from  those  of  the  control  (Fig.  2B, 
before  size  fractionation).  After  passage  of  the  methanol 
eluate  through  the  10  kDa  and  1  kDa  membranes,  activity 
reappeared  (Fig.  2B),  presumably  due  to  the  removal  of 
an  inhibitor  introduced  by,  or  concentrated  by,  the  resin 
(see  Discussion).  In  these  two  fractions,  an  increase  in  the 
percentage  of  response  with  concentration  was  observed, 
with  concentrations  >  10  larvae/ml  producing  levels  of 
response  significantly  different  from  control.  The  fractions 
from  these  two  nitrations,  which  were  retained  above  the 
membranes  (the  >10  kDa  and  the  <10  kDa  but  >1  kDa 
fractions),  produced  levels  of  response  no  different  from 
controls  at  concentrations  up  to  20  larvae/ml. 

When  untreated  hatch  water  was  passed  through  the 
10  kDa  cutoff  membrane,  the  retained  >  10  kDa  fraction 
lacked  biological  activity  (Fig.  4).  Response  levels  in  the 


8765432          I 
Days   until    Hatching 

Figure  1.  Frequency  of  spontaneous  abdomen  pumping  (A),  and 
percentage  of  crabs  which  pumped  (B),  as  a  function  of  embryo  age  in 
ovigerous  female  Neopamtpc  sari  Means  and  95%  confidence  limits  of 
spontaneous  pumping  frequency  are  shown  for  crabs  that  pumped  at 
least  once.  The  numbers  beside  the  points  are  the  sample  sizes. 


<10  kDa  fraction  were  significantly  greater  than  control 
levels  at  >  10  larvae/ml.  The  absence  of  biological  activity 
in  the  >  10  kDa  fraction  of  the  untreated  hatch  water  sug- 
gests that  precipitation  of  large  (>10  kDa),  biologically 
active  molecules  onto  the  resin  upon  methanol  elution 
was  unimportant.  In  summary,  pumping  factors  were  ad- 
sorbed to  XAD-7  resin,  eluted  with  methanol,  and  were 
<1  kDa. 

Controls  demonstrated  that  biologically  active  sub- 
stances originate  from  hatching  eggs.  Filtered  seawater 
and  ovigerous  crab  essence  were  subjected  to  the  adsorp- 
tion chromatograph  and  fractionation  procedures.  At 
concentrations  equivalent  to  20  and  50  larvae/ml,  neither 
filtered  seawater  nor  crab  essence  produced  responses  sig- 
nificantly greater  than  the  filtered  seawater  control,  either 
before  or  after  passage  of  the  two  solutions  through  the 
resin  and  membranes  (Table  II).  These  results  show  that 
passage  of  seawater  through  the  resin  and  membranes  did 
not  add  excitatory  substances  to  the  water,  and  that  sub- 
stances associated  with  ovigerous  females,  in  the  absence 
of  egg  hatching,  did  not  produce  a  significant  level  of 
pumping  response. 


M.  C.   DE   VRIES  KT  AL 


70 
60 

50- 
40 
30 
20 
10- 

70- 
60 
50- 
40- 
30 
20- 
10- 


A   Neoponope  soy/ 
«59 


B.    Neoponope  sayi 


«  <l  KD    n=30 


<IOkD    n=23-30 


„  -  -*  before  size  f  ro> 
n  =  60 

\'J'  "'•-.,....-•>  10  kD  n  =23-25 

''•'-+-—-"  "'"•  <IOkDbut>l  kD    n  = 


23-29 


.Control 
n  =  !58 


15  20  25  30  35 

Concentration     (larvae/ml) 


40         60 


Figure  2.  Percentage  of  ovigerous  female  Neopanope  sari  that  responded  to  N.  sari  hatch  water  (A)  and 
to  various  size  fractions  of  a  methanol  eluate  of  hatch  water  after  concentration  on  XAD-7  resin  (B).  Con- 
centration of  active  substances  (abscissa)  was  calculated  from  estimates  of  larval  concentration  in  the  untreated 
hatch  water  solutions.  Numbers  by  points  are  specific  sample  sizes  (A)  or  a  range  of  sample  sizes  (B).  Asterisks 
indicate  the  first  concentration  at  which  pumping  response  was  significantly  different  from  controls.  Control 
levels  for  pumping  response  were  established  in  filtered  seawater.  Crabs  with  late  embryos  were  0-3  days 
from  larval  release,  and  those  with  early  embrvos  were  >5  davs  from  larval  release. 


Amino  acid  analysis 

Reverse-phase  HPLC  analysis  of  free  and  hydrolyzable 
amino  acids  showed  picomolar  concentrations  of  15 
amino  acids  in  the  biologically  active  (<1  kDa)  fraction 
of  hatch  water  concentrated  on  the  resin  [calculated  to  a 
liter  of  160  larvae/ml  for  comparison  with  results  in 
Rittschof  et  al.  (1985);  Table  III].  The  most  abundant 
amino  acids  after  hydrolysis — cysteine,  glycine,  isoleucine, 
and  methionine — accounted  for  47%  of  the  total  free,  and 
57%  of  the  total  hydrolyzable  amino  acids.  These  four 
amino  acids  were  approximately  equimolar,  at  >  100  pA/ 
after  acid  hydrolysis.  Free  amino  acids  before  hydrolysis 
represented  23%  of  the  total  amino  acids  in  the  sample 
after  hydrolysis.  N.  sayi  pumping  factors  of  this  liter  thus 


conlain  picomolar  amounls  of  amino  acids,  mosl  of  which 
appear  lo  be  bound  in  peplides. 

Amino  acid  experiments 

When  presenled  wilh  mixlures  of  Ihe  amino  acids  mosl 
abundant  in  partially  purified  halch  waler  from  N.  sayi 
and  R.  hanisii,  ovigerous  N.  sayi  individuals  significantly 
increased  Iheir  levels  of  pumping  over  Ihose  of  controls, 
al  concentralions  >  10~4  M  (Fig.  5).  Concenlralions  al 
which  nalive  pumping  factors  were  effeclive  (10~7-10~9 
M),  produced  pumping  responses  no  differenl  from  con- 
Irols.  These  effeclive  concenlralions  are  based  on  those 
of  Ihe  four  major  amino  acids  (Table  III)  in  halch  waler 
of  tiler  >  2.5  larvae/ml  (i.e..  Ihe  Ihreshold  for  crabs  car- 
rying lale  embryos;  Fig.  2).  In  conlrasl,  amino  acid  mix- 


CONTROL  OF  CRAB  LARVAL  RELEASE 


70 


50 


rr 


30 


10 


Neopanope  sayi 
n  =  27-3l 


Rhtfhroponopeus  narrtsit 
hotch  water  . 


Uca  pugitator 
hotch  water 


-  •—  _^  Sesarma  cinereum 

-"---_       hatch  water 


Control 
n=!58 


30  50 

Concentration  (larvae/ml) 


70 


Figure  3.  Percentage  of  ovigerous  female  A  Vi  >/><(/;<>/>(•  xayi  with  late 
embryos  that  responded  to  hatch  water  from  three  other  brachyurans: 
Rhillm  tpanopent  liarrisii.  T.  </  pux/lutor,  and  Scsamui  cincrcuni  Asterisks 
indicate  the  first  concentration  at  which  the  pumping  response  was  sig- 
nificantly different  from  controls.  Control  levels  for  pumping  response 
were  established  in  filtered  seawater. 


tures  first  produced  significant  responses  at  concentrations 
corresponding  to  approximately  100,000  larvae/ml  (2.5 
X  10~4  A/)  for  the  N.  sayi  mixture,  and  approximately 
7,000  larvae/ml  (8.3  X  10~4  M)  for  the  R.  liarrisii  mixture. 
These  results  suggest  that  simple  mixtures  of  the  amino 
acids  most  abundant  in  pumping  factors  are  not  the  mol- 
ecules most  active  in  producing  abdomen  pumping  at  the 
time  of  larval  release. 

Discussion 

Substances  associated  with  hatching  eggs  evoked  ste- 
reotyped larval  release  behaviors  in  ovigerous  females  of 
the  crab  Neopanope  sayi.  Responsiveness  to  these  pump- 
ing factors  varied  with  embryo  age.  as  did  the  spontaneous 
pumping  activity  of  ovigerous  crabs.  The  pumping  factors 
adsorbed  to  Amberlite  XAD-7  resin  and  could  be  eluted 
with  methanol,  but  biological  activity  did  not  appear  in 
the  methanol  eluate  until  after  size  fractionation.  The 
presence  of  small  peptides  was  inferred  from  the  size  frac- 
tionation and  amino  acid  analysis  of  a  partially  purified 
preparation  of  hatch  water. 

For  N.  sayi,  a  clear  variation  was  observed  in  the  fre- 
quency of  spontaneous  abdomen  pumping  with  embryo 
age.  A  possible  physiological  explanation  for  this  phe- 
nomenon is  that,  as  nonliving  yolk  is  converted  into  em- 
bryo, metabolic  rate  increases,  causing  increased  Oi  de- 
mand and  waste  production.  Abdomen  pumping  by  crabs 
is  thought  to  facilitate  O2  transport  to,  and  waste  removal 
from,  developing  embryos  (Templeman,  1937;  Ennis, 
1973),  hence  older  embryos  with  higher  metabolic  rates 
would  require  more  water  pumped  around  them. 

In  contrast  to  those  of  N.  sayi,  the  spontaneous  abdo- 
men pumping  rates  of  R.  harrisii  were  independent  of 


Table  I 

Effect  til  IHISMHK  lunch  miler  ilmiitgh  Anjher/ile  \AD-7  resin 


Test 

concentration 

Percentage 

Fraction 

larvae/ml 

n 

responding 

Hatch  water  before  resin 

10 

59 

69 

Hatch  water  after  resin 

10 

30 

33 

Control 

— 

158 

23 

embryo  age  (Forward  and  Lohmann,  1983).  This  species 
difference  may  be  due  in  part  to  the  smaller  size  of  R 
harrisii  egg  masses  (average  size,  about  1000  eggs;  For- 
ward, unpub.  data)  compared  with  N.  sayi  (generally 
2000-4000  eggs;  De  Vries  unpub.  data).  Eggs  of  the  two 
species  are  of  approximately  the  same  diameter  [about 
400  urn  on  the  day  of  hatching  (De  Vries  and  Forward, 
1990b;  De  Vries,  unpub.  data)].  Therefore,  the  egg  masses 
of  N.  sayi  are  2-4  times  larger  in  volume  than  those  of 
R  liarrisii.  and  the  former  would  have  a  greater  total 
metabolic  demand  and  slower  diffusion  rate  of  water 
through  the  egg  mass.  These  effects  may  compound  one 
another,  leading  to  a  much  greater  need  for  water  transport 
around  A',  sayi  eggs,  and  thus  a  more  pronounced  increase 
in  pumping  rate  as  the  embryos  mature.  The  increase  in 
egg  mass  size  (De  Vries  and  Forward,  1990b)  with  embryo 
age  may  cause  the  increase  in  spontaneous  pumping  rate 
by  stimulating  stretch  receptors. 

The  response  of  N.  sayi  to  hatch  water  of  conspecifics 
is  similar  to  that  observed  previously  for  R.  harrisii  (For- 


50- 


CD 

rr 


30- 


10- 


Neopanope  sayi 
n= 30-60 


/<  10  kd  N.  sayi  hatch  water 


.Control 

n=l58 


>IOkd  N.  sayi  hatch  water 


10  20 

Concentration  (larvae/ml) 


30 


Figure  -4.  Percentage  of  ovigerous  females  of  Neopanope  sayi  with 
late  embryos  that  responded  to  the  retained  and  non-retained  fractions 
o(N.  suvi  hatch  water  after  ultrafiltration  with  a  10  kDa  membrane.  The 
control  is  for  responses  in  filtered  seawater. 


M.  C.  DE  VRIES  ET  AL 
Table  II 


Controls  performed  for  Neopanope  sayi  pumpiiiK  factor  cx 


Solution 

Test  concentration 
(larvae/ml) 

n 

Percentage 
responding 

1.  0.45  Mm  filtered  seawater  (FSW)  (untreated)  (control) 

— 

158 

23 

2.   10  kDa  fraction  MeOH  eluate  FSW 

2(1 

31 

23 

10  kDa  fraction  MeOH  eluate  FSW 

50 

30 

17 

3.    1  kDa  fraction  MeOH  eluate  FSW 

20 

30 

17 

I  kDa  fraction  MeOH  eluate  FSW 

50 

30 

17 

4.  Ovigerous  crab  essence  (OCE)  (untreated) 

5    10  kDa  fraction  MeOH  eluate  OCE 
1 0  kDa  fraction  MeOH  eluate  OCE 

6.   1  kDa  fraction  MeOH  eluate  OCE 
1  kDa  fraction  MeOH  eluate  OCE 


25 

20 
50 

20 
50 


25 

35 
35 

35 
35 


13 

26 
17 

23 
6 


In  all  tests,  crabs  cam-ing  embryos  within  three  days  of  hatching  were  used.  Test  concentrations  were  calculated  as  described  in  the  text.  Solutions 
2-6  produced  response  rates  not  significantly  greater  than  control  (Solution  1 ).  Ovigerous  crab  essence  was  seawater  in  which  ovigerous  crabs  had 
been  incubated  for  several  hours  (details  in  text). 


ward  and  Lohmann,  1983),  which  suggests  that  the  model 
for  hatching-time  control  described  for  R.  harrisii  also 
applies  for  N.  sayi.  In  this  model,  synchronized  devel- 
opment of  the  eggs  is  believed  to  result  from  an  unknown 
interaction  between  the  embryos  and  the  females,  but  ac- 
tual hatching  time  is  controlled  by  the  embryos.  When 
the  eggs  become  ready  to  hatch,  several  eggs  hatch  spon- 
taneously, releasing  substances  into  the  water  that  stim- 
ulate additional  abdomen  pumping.  This  action  breaks 
open  the  remaining  eggs,  and  the  result  is  the  synchronous 
release  of  larvae.  The  release  of  pheromones  by  embryos 
to  communicate  their  readiness  to  hatch  may  have  adap- 
tive significance.  During  larval  release,  female  crabs  are 
presumably  exposed  and  therefore  at  greater  risk  to  pre- 
dation  than  at  other  times  (Forward  and  Lohmann,  1983). 
In  addition,  abdominal  pumping  during  larval  release  is 
a  vigorous,  hence  an  energetically  costly  activity.  Con- 
centration of  larval  release  behaviors  to  a  time  when  the 
embryos  are  ready  to  hatch  may  thus  decrease  the  risk  of 
predation  and  energetic  costs  to  the  female. 

Substances  (pumping  factors)  that  evoke  larval  release 
behavior  (pumping  response)  were  released  at  the  time  of 
egg  hatching  and  were  not  released  from  eggs  prior  to  this 
time.  The  effects  of  different  treatments  with  these  pump- 
ing factors  can  be  determined  by  comparing  the  effective 
concentrations  that  evoked  a  50%  pumping  response 
(EC50;  Table  IV).  When  exposed  to  untreated  hatch  water, 
the  liter  for  the  EC50  was  6  larvae/ml.  If  this  water  was 
passed  through  a  10  kDa  cutoff  membrane,  the  EC50  in- 
creased to  10  larvae/ml.  Because  activity  could  not  be 
detected  in  the  water  above  the  10  kDa  membrane,  this 
decrease  in  activity  did  not  result  from  the  removal  of 
large  active  molecules.  Two  possible  explanations  are  ad- 


sorption of  active  molecules  to  the  membrane  during  the 
filtration  process,  or  degradation  during  the  filtration  in- 
terval by  enzymes  released  by  lysed  microorganisms  dur- 
ing thawing  and  pressure  dialysis  of  hatch  water.  Crabs 
were  unresponsive  to  hatch  water  that  had  been  passed 
through  the  X  AD- 7  resin,  suggesting  that  active  substances 

Table  III 

Amino  acid  composition  of  hydrolyzed  and  unhydrolyied  Neopanope 
sayi  pumping  factor  of  <  1 000  daltons 

Picomolar  amount 


Amino  Acid 

Unhydrolyzed 
factor 

Hydrolyzed 
factor 

Difference 

Pro 

_ 

35 

35 

Cys 

53 

134 

SI 

Asp 

— 

13 

13 

Thr 

6 

29 

23 

Ser 

15 

61 

46 

Glu 

— 

47 

47 

Gly 

6 

125 

119 

Ala 

6 

58 

52 

Val 

28 

61 

33 

Met 

3 

137 

134 

lie 

43 

123 

80 

Leu 

4 

34 

30 

Tyr 

— 

— 

— 

Phe 

51 

54 

3 

His 

— 

— 

— 

Lys 

7 

15 

8 

Arg 

— 

19 

19 

Total 

2?7 

945 

723 

Concentrations  are  calculated  for  a  sample  with  a  titer  of  160  larvae/ 


ml. 


CONTROL  OF  CRAB  LARVAL   RLLF.ASE 


were  removed  by  the  column.  The  methanol  eluate  was 
also  inactive.  Pressure  dialysis  of  the  methanol  eluate  re- 
sulted in  a  titer  of  activity  comparable  to  size  fractionated 
(10  kd  filtered)  hatch  water  that  had  not  been  passed 
through  the  resin.  This  result  suggests  high  recovery  of 
pumping  factors  from  the  column,  as  previously  obtained 
for  R  harrisii  factors  (Rittschof  ct  ul..  1985). 

The  apparent  absence  of  activity  in  the  methanol  eluate 
may  have  resulted  because  the  resin:  (1)  removed  an  im- 
portant component  of  the  pumping  factors  that  was  not 
eluted  with  methanol;  (2)  concentrated  high  molecular 
weight  inhibitory  substances  in  the  hatch  water;  or  (3) 
added  inhibitory  substances  dc  novo.  The  column  did  not 
add  inhibitory  molecules  because  XAD-7  resin  leaches 
low  molecular  weight  compounds  ( Jolley  el  ai,  1981)  that 
would  not  be  removed  by  size  fractionation.  Because  ac- 
tivity reappeared  after  passage  through  the  10  kDa  mem- 
brane (Fig.  4),  the  active  factors  and  components  were 
recovered  from  the  resin.  Thus,  the  most  likely  explana- 
tion is  that  inhibition  resulted  from  concentration  of  high 
molecular  weight  inhibitory  substances  present  in  the 
hatch  water. 

The  EC5o  for  the  methanol  eluate  after  passage  through 
the  10  kDa  membrane  was  13  larvae/ml  (Table  IV).  Be- 
cause this  value  is  very  close  to  that  for  untreated  hatch 
water  (10  larvae/ml)  after  passage  through  the  10  kDa 


Effective  ciineenlruli(iii\  lor  a  5 
carrying  lalc-slage  embryos 


Table  IV 

0%  pumping  response  (E(.'SII)  in  cnihs 


Hatch  water  treatment 

EC50 
(larvae/ml) 

Data  source 

Untreated 

6 

Fig.  2 

10  kDa  filtered 

10 

Fig.  4 

XAD-7  resin 

none 

Fig.  2 

XAD-7  resin.  10  kDa 

filtered 

13 

Fig.  2 

XAD-7  resin,  10  kDa. 

1  kDa  filtered 

12 

Fig.  2 

Rhilhropanopeus  hamsn 

hatch  water 

15 

Fig.  3 

Veil  pugilator  hatch  water 

50 

Fig.  3 

membrane,  recovery  of  active  molecules  from  the  XAD- 
7  column  was  close  to  100%.  Rittschof  el  ai  (1985)  had 
similar  success  rates  in  recovery  of  active  molecules  in  R. 
harrisii  hatch  water  from  an  XAD-7  column.  The  activity 
of  N.  xayi  hatch  water  remained  the  same  (EC50  =  12 
larvae/ml)  after  it  had  been  passed  through  the  1  kDa 
membrane,  indicating  that  activity  can  be  attributed  solely 
to  molecules  that  are  less  than  1  kDa  in  size. 

N.  sari  responded  to  hatch  water  from  R.  harrisii  and 
Uca  piigilalor,  but  the  EC50  values  were  high  at  1 5  and 


60 


50 


<1> 
(f> 
c  40 

CL 
0> 

01   30 


20 


10 


Neopanope  say/ 
n=29-34 


N  soy/  mixture 
cystglytile  +  met 
(1=1=1=1 


Control 
n  =  !58 


Rhithroponopeus  harrisii  mixture 

glytorg 

(LI) 


10 


.-9 


ICT 


'-7 


10"  10  10  10" 

Concentration  (M) 


10' 


,-3 


10' 


Figure  5.  Percentage  of  ovigerous  Neopanope  sayi  with  late  embryos  that  responded  to  different  con- 
centrations of  mixtures  ot  amino  acids.  Mixtures  represent  the  main  components  of  hydrolyzed  pumping 
factors  of  N.  sayi  (equimolar  amounts  of  L-cysteine,  glycine.  L-isoleucine,  and  L-metlnonine)  and  Rhilh- 
ropanopeus  harrisii  (equimolar  amounts  of  glycine  and  L-arginine;  Rittschof  el  ul.,  1985).  Asterisks  indicate 
the  first  concentration  at  which  the  pumping  response  was  significantly  different  from  controls.  Control 
levels  for  the  response  were  established  in  filtered  seawater. 


10 


M.  C.  DE  VRIES  ET  AL 


50  larvae/ml,  respectively  (Table  IV).  This  result,  and  the 
lack  of  response  to  S.  cinerewn  hatch  water,  show  that 
cross-reactivity  among  species  occurs,  but  is  incomplete, 
implying  that  hatch  waters  from  different  species  may  be 
similar,  but  are  probably  not  identical. 

Three  lines  of  evidence  suggest  that  the  active  molecules 
in  hatch  water  of  R.  harrisii  are  peptides.  First,  activity 
is  lost  if  hatch  water  is  treated  with  a  protease.  Second, 
compositional  analysis  of  partially  purified  active  mole- 
cules suggests  that  they  may  consist  of  di-  or  tripeptides 
with  a  neutral  amino  acid  at  the  amino-terminus  and  an 
arginine  carboxy-terminus  (Rittschof  et  at.,  1985).  Third, 
pure  peptides  having  this  structure  induce  pumping  re- 
sponses (Forward  et  a/..  1987;  Rittschof  et  ai,  1989). 

Results  from  the  present  study  support  the  hypothesis 
that  active  substances  in  hatch  water  of  N.  sayi  are  also 
peptides,  though  their  exact  chemical  nature  is  unknown. 
Evidence  for  this  includes:  (1)  responsiveness  of  N.  sayi 
to  R.  harrisii  hatch  water;  (2)  general  similarity  in  the 
adsorptive  characteristics  of  N.  sayi  and  R.  harrisii 
pumping  factors  toward  XAD-7  resin;  (3)  suggestion  of 
peptides  in  a  partially  purified  hatch  water  preparation; 
and  (4)  similarity  in  the  response  of  N.  sayi  and  R.  harrisii 
to  mixtures  of  amino  acids. 

The  amino  acid  analysis  of  Neopanope  sayi  pumping 
factors  indicates  that  the  four  main  amino  acids  in  the 
proposed  peptides  are  cysteine,  glycine,  isoleucine,  and 
methionine.  With  the  exception  of  glycine,  these  amino 
acids  have  neutral  side  chains  and  are  hydrophobic.  In 
contrast,  the  proposed  active  peptides  of  Rhithropanopeus 
harrisii  contain  arginine  (present  only  in  low  amounts  in 
N.  sayi  factor),  which  is  strongly  charged  and  hydrophilic. 
Thus  the  foregoing  analysis  suggests  that  the  pumping 
factors  of  N.  sayi  are  more  hydrophobic  than  those  of 
R.  harrisii. 

Bioassays  of  amino  acids  suggest  that,  for  both  species, 
simple  mixtures  of  amino  acids  are  not  the  active  com- 
ponents of  pumping  factors  (Rittschof  et  ai.  1985).  Mix- 
tures of  the  amino  acids  most  abundant  in  the  pumping 
factors  produced  significant  levels  of  pumping  only  at 
concentrations  much  higher  than  those  present  in  native 
hatch  waters.  The  threshold  concentrations  for  responses 
of  N.  sayi  to  amino  acids  were  about  10~4  M.  while  free 
amino  acid  levels  based  on  compositional  analysis  (Table 
III)  at  the  threshold  concentration  of  hatch  water  (2.5 
larvae/ml;  Fig.  2)  were  about  3.0  X  10~12  M  (calculated 
from  Table  III).  For  TV.  sayi,  small  peptides  of  undeter- 
mined sequence  are  hypothesized  to  be  the  active  com- 
ponents of  pumping  factors  as  concluded  for  R.  harrisii 
(Rittschof  et  al.,  1985).  The  source  of  these  peptides  is  as 
yet  unknown.  However  we  have  postulated  elsewhere  that 
enzymatic  degradation  of  the  egg  membranes  (De  Vries 
and  Forward,  1 99  Ib;  Rittschof  el  al..  1990a)  produces  the 
pumping  factors. 


The  chemical  mediation  of  a  diversity  of  behaviors  has 
been  described  in  virtually  all  phyla  and  may  be  partic- 
ularly important  in  aquatic  environments  (e.g.,  Knight- 
Jones,  1953;  Collins,  1975;  Trott  and  Dimock.  1978; 
Derby  and  Atema,  1980;  Tierney  and  Dunham.  1982; 
Rittschof  el  al..  1983,  1984).  In  particular,  proteins  and 
peptides  elicit  behaviors  in  various  taxa,  including  feeding 
behavior  in  the  snail  Ilyanassa  (=Nassarius)  obsoleta 
(Carr  et  al.,  1974),  creeping  in  predatory  snails  (Rittschof 
et  a/.,  1984),  and  metamorphosis  in  the  sand  dollar  Den- 
drastcr  excentricus  (Burke,  1984)  and  the  abalone  Haliotis 
(Morse,  1988).  Among  crustaceans,  serine  protease  gen- 
erated peptides  are  implicated  in  hermit  crab  shell  ac- 
quisition behavior  (Rittschof,  1980;  Lepore  and  Gilchrist, 
1988;  Rittschof  et  al..  1990b),  barnacle  attachment  be- 
havior (Rittschof,  1985)  and  metamorphosis  (Tegtmeyer 
and  Rittschof,  1989),  and  crustacean  larval  release  (Ritts- 
chof et  al..  1990a).  Rittschof  and  Bonaventura  (1986)  ar- 
gue that  distinct  advantages  are  inherent  in  the  use  of 
peptides  as  chemical  cues  in  aquatic  systems,  including: 
( 1 )  increased  complexity  of  primary  structure  (compared 
to  amino  acids),  allowing  opportunity  for  increased  re- 
sponse specificity;  (2)  background  concentrations  in  ma- 
rine systems  are  low  (Mopper  and  Lindroth,  1982),  al- 
lowing for  high  signal  to  noise  ratios;  and  (3)  metabolic 
inexpensiveness,  because  they  need  not  be  synthesized  de 
novo.  but  may  be  broken  down  from  existing  structural 
and  metabolic  components. 

Acknowledgments 

tt 

We  thank  Dr.  D.  Fiorio  for  performing  the  amino  acid 
analysis  of  TV.  sayi  pumping  factors.  We  are  grateful  to  C. 
Buswell,  K.  Eisenman,  E.  Herzog,  S.  Posey,  M.  Wach- 
owiak  and  C.  Wellins  for  technical  assistance.  This  ma- 
terial is  based  on  research  supported  by  the  National  Sci- 
ence Foundation  under  Grants  No.  OCE-8603945  and 
DCB-8701544. 

Literature  Cited 

Branford,  J.  R.  1978.  The  influence  of  daylength,  temperature  and 
season  on  the  hatching  rhythm  of  Homanis gamntanis. ./.  Mar.  Biol. 
ASXM:  L'.K.  58:  639-658. 

Burke,  R.  D.  1984.  Pheromonal  control  of  metamorphosis  in  the  Pacific 
sand  dollar.  Dendraster  excentricus.  Science  225:  442-443. 

Carr,  W.  E.  S.,  E.  R.  Hall,  and  S.  Gurin.  1974.  Chemoreception  and 
the  role  of  proteins,  a  comparative  study.  Comp.  Biochem.  Physio! 
47A:  559-566. 

Christy,  J.  H.  1986.  Timing  of  larval  release  by  intertidal  crabs  on  an 
exposed  shore.  Bull.  Mar.  Sci.  39:  176-191. 

Collins,  A.  R.  S.  1975.  Biochemical  investigation  of  two  responses  in- 
volved in  the  feeding  behavior  of  Acanthaster planci  (L.)  II.  Isolation 
and  characterization  of  chemical  stimuli.  J  Exp.  Mar.  Biol  Ecol. 
17:  69-86. 

DeCoursey,  P.  J.  1979.  Egg  hatching  rhythms  in  three  species  of  fiddler 
crabs.  Pp.  399-406  in  Cyclic  Phenomena  in  Marine  Plants  and  An- 
imals. E.  Naylor  and  R.  G.  Hartnoll,  eds.  Pergamon  Press.  Oxford. 


CONTROL  OF  CRAB  LARVAL  RELEASE 


1  I 


DeCoursey,  P.  J.  1983.  Biological  timing.  Pp.  107-162  in  The  Biology 
of  Crustacea,  Vol.  7,  F.  J.  Vernherg  and  W.  B.  Vernberg,  eds.  Aca- 
demic Press,  New  York. 

Derby,  C.  D.,  and  J.  Atema.  1980.  Induced  host  odor  attraction  in  the 
pea  crab  Pinnotheres  maeulaliis.  Biol.  Bull.  158:  26-33. 

De  Vries,  M.  C.  1990.  Control  of  egg  hatching  in  crabs:  a  comparative 
study  of  species  from  different  tidal  heights.  Ph.D.  dissertation.  Duke 
Univ.,  Durham,  NC. 

De  Vries,  M.  O,  and  R.  B.  Forward  Jr.  1989.  Rhythms  in  larval  release 
of  the  sublittoral  crab  Neopanope  sayi  and  the  supralittoral  crab  Se- 
sarma  cincrcum  Mar.  Bio/.  100:  241-248. 

De  Vries,  M.  C.,  and  R.  B.  Forward  Jr.  1991a.  Control  of  egg-hatching 
time  in  crabs  from  different  tidal  heights.  J  Crust.  Biol.  In  press. 

De  Vries,  M.  C.,  and  R.  B.  Forward  Jr.  1991b.  Mechanisms  of  crus- 
tacean egg  hatching:  evidence  for  enzyme  release  by  crab  embryos. 
Mar.  Biol. 

Ennis,  G.  P.  1973.  Endogenous  rhythmicity  associated  with  larval 
hatching  in  the  lobster  Homarus  gammartts.  J.  Mar.  Biol.  Assoc. 
I '.A.'.  53:  531-538. 

Forward,  R.  B.,  Jr.  1987.  Larval  release  rhythms  of  decapod  crusta- 
ceans: an  overview.  Bull.  Mar.  Sci  41:  165-176. 

Forward,  R.  B.,  Jr.,  and  K.  J.  Lohmann.  1983.  Control  of  egg  hatching 
in  the  crab  Rhithropanopeus  harrisii  Biol.  Bull  165:  154-166. 

Forward,  R.  B.,  Jr.,  K.  J.  Lohmann.  and  T.  \V.  Cronin.  1982.  Rhythms 
in  larval  release  by  an  estuarine  crab  (Rhithropanopeus  harrisii).  Biol. 
Bull  163:  2X7-300. 

Forward,  R.  B.,  Jr.,  D.  Rittschof.  and  M.  C.  De  Vries.  1987.  Peptide 
pheromones  synchronize  crustacean  egg  hatching  and  larval  release. 
Chem.  Seines  12:  491-498. 

Henrikson,  R.  L.,  and  S.  C.  Meredith.  1984.  Amino  acid  analysis  by 
reverse-phase  high-performance  liquid  chromatography:  precolumn 
derivatization  with  phenyhsothiocyanate.  Anal  Biochem,  136:  65- 
74. 

Jolley,  R.  L.  1981 .  Concentrating  organics  in  water  for  biological  testing. 
Environ.  Sci.  Technol.  158:  874-880. 

Lepore,  M.,  and  S.  Gilchrist.  1988.  Hermit  crab  attraction  to  gastropod 
predation  sites.  Am.  Zoo/.  28(4):  93A. 

Knight-Jones,  G.  W.  1953.  Laboratory  experiments  on  greganousness 
during  setting  in  Balamis  balanoidcs  and  other  barnacles.  J  Exp. 
Biol.  30:  584-598. 

Mopper,  K.,  and  P.  Lindroth.  1982.  Diel  and  depth  variations  in  dis- 
solved free  amino  acids  and  ammonium  in  the  Baltic  Sea  determined 
by  shipboard  HPLC  analysis.  Limnol.  Oeeanogr.  27:  336-337. 

Morse,  A.  1988.  The  role  of  algal  metabolites  in  the  recruitment  process. 
Pp.  463-475  in  Marine  Biodeterioration:  Advanced  Technique*  Ap- 
plicable to  the  Indian  Ocean.  M.  Thompson  and  R.  S.  Nagahhu- 
shanam.  eds.  Oxford  &  IBW.  Bombay. 

Pandian,  T.  J.  1970.  Ecophysiological  studies  on  the  developing  eggs 
and  embryos  of  the  European  lobster  Homarus  gammarus.  Afar. 
Biol  5:  154-167. 

Rittschof,  D.  1980.  Enzymatic  production  of  small  molecules  attracting 
hermit  crabs  to  simulated  gastropod  predation  sites.  J.  Chem.  Ecol. 
6:  665-675. 


Rittschof,  D.  1985.  Oyster  drills  and  the  frontiers  of  chemical  ecology: 
unsettling  ideas.  Am.  Malucolog.  Bull .  Spec.  Ed.  1:  I  I  1-1  16. 

Rittschof,  D.,  and  J.  Bonaventura.  1986.  Macromolecular  cues  in  ma- 
rine systems.  /  Chem.  Ecol.  12:  1013-1023. 

Rittschof,  D.,  L.  G.  Williams,  B.  Brown,  and  M.  R.  Carriker. 
1983.  Chemical  attraction  of  newly  hatched  oyster  drills.  Biol.  Bull. 
164:  493-505. 

Rittschof,  D.,  D.  keiber,  and  C.  Merrill.  1984.  Modification  of  responses 
of  newly  hatched  snails  by  exposure  to  odors  during  development. 
Chem.  Senses  9:  181-192. 

Rittschof,  D.,  R.  B.  Forward  Jr.,  and  D.  D.  Mott.  1985.  Larval  release 
in  the  crab  Rlulhropunopeus  harrisii  (Gould):  chemical  cues  from 
hatching  eggs.  Chem.  Senses  10:  567-577. 

Rittschof,  D.,  R.  B.  Forward  Jr.,  D.  A.  Simons,  P.  Amanlha  Reddy,  and 
B.  W.  Erickson.  1989.  Peptide  analogs  of  the  mud  crab  pumping 
pheromone.  Chem  Senses  14:  137-148. 

Rittschof,  D.,  R.  B.  Forward  Jr.,  and  B.  W.  Erickson.  1990a.  Larval 
release  in  brachyuran  crustaceans:  functional  similarity  of  the  peptide 
receptor  and  the  catalytic  site  of  trypsm.  J.  Chem.  Ecol  16:  1359- 
1370. 

Rittschof,  D.,  C.  M.  Kratt,  and  A.  S.  Clare.  1990b.  Gastropod  predation 
sites:  the  role  of  predator  and  prey  in  chemical  attraction  of  the  hermit 
crab  Clibananus  villains  (Bosc).  JMBA  70:  583-596. 

Saigusa.  M.  1981.  Adaptive  significance  of  semilunar  rhythm  in  the 
terrestrial  crab  Sesarma.  Biol  Bull  160:  31 1-321. 

Saigusa,  M.  1982.  Larval  release  rhythm  coinciding  with  solar  day  and 
tidal  cycles  in  the  terrestrial  crab  Sesarma — harmony  with  the  semi- 
lunar  timing  and  its  adaptive  significance.  Biol.  Bull.  162:  371-386. 

Saigusa,  M.,  and  T.  llidaka.  1978.  Semilunar  rhythms  in  zoeae-release 
activity  of  the  land  crab  Sesarma.  Oecologia  37:  163-176. 

Salmon,  M.,  W.  H.  Seiple,  and  S.  G.  Morgan.  1986.  Hatching  rhythms 
of  fiddler  crabs  and  associated  species  at  Beaufort,  North  Carolina. 
J.  Crust.  Biol  6:  24-36. 

Tegtmeyer,  K.,  and  D.  Rittschof.  1989.  Synthetic  peptide  analogs  to 
barnacle  settlement  pheromone.  Peptides9:  1403-1406. 

Templeman,  W.  1937.  Egg-laying  and  hatching  postures  and  habits  of 
the  American  lobster  (Homarus  americanus).  J  Eish  Res.  Bd.  Can- 
ada 3:  339-342. 

Tierney,  A.  J.,  and  D.  VV.  Dunham.  1982.  Chemical  communication 
in  the  reproductive  isolation  of  the  crayfishes  Orconecles  propinqus 
and  Orconectes  vinlis  (Decapoda.  Cambaridae).  /  Crust.  Biol.  2: 
544_548. 

Trott,  T.  J.,  and  R.  V.  Dimock.  1978.  Intraspecific  trail  following  by 
the  mud  snail  Ilvanassu  ohsuleta.  .Mar.  Behav  Physiol,  5:  91-101. 

VValpole,  R.  E.  1974.     Introduction  to  Statistics.  Macmillan.  New  York. 

Williams,  A.  B.  1984.  Shrimps.  Lobsters,  and  Crabs  of  the  Atlantic 
Coast  of  the  Eastern  United  States,  Maine  to  Florida.  Smithsonian 
Institution  Press,  Washington  DC. 

W'olcott,  T.  W.,  and  D.  L.  Wolcott.  1982.  Larval  loss  and  spawning 
behavior  in  the  land  crab  Gecarcinus  lateralis  (Fremenville).  J.  Crust. 
Biol.  2:  477-485. 


Reference:  Biol.  Bull  180:  12-27.  (February.  1991) 


Particle  Captures  and  the  Method  of  Suspension 
Feeding  by  Echinoderm  Larvae 


MICHAEL  W.  HART 

Department  of  Zoology.  NJ-15.  University  of  Washington,  Scuttle.  Washington.  98195  and 
Friday  Harbor  Laboratories,  620  University  Road,  Friday  Harbor.  Washington.  98250 


Abstract.  Motivated  by  discrepancies  between  two  re- 
cent descriptions  of  the  suspension-feeding  mechanism 
employed  by  echinoderm  larvae,  I  describe  particle  cap- 
tures by  the  larvae  of  seven  species  of  temperate  eastern 
Pacific  echinoderms  from  four  classes.  When  videotape 
recordings  of  free-swimming  larvae  clearing  plastic  spheres 
from  suspension  were  analyzed,  two  modes  of  particle 
capture  were  observed  to  operate.  The  majority  of  cap- 
tured spheres  were  caught  at  the  peripheral  ciliated  band 
and  then  transported  to  the  mouth,  often  by  repeated 
capture  on  portions  of  the  band  progressively  nearer  to 
the  mouth.  This  description  is  consistent  with  the  ciliary 
reversal  model  of  suspension  feeding  described  by  R.  R. 
Strathmann.  A  small  minority  of  captured  spheres  fol- 
lowed broad,  curving  paths  directly  into  the  larval  mouth 
without  interception  at  the  ciliated  band.  These  particle 
paths  resemble  those  described  by  T.  H.  J.  Gilmour.  The 
videotape  recordings  also  permitted  a  quantitative  com- 
parison of  suspension  feeding  by  these  larvae.  Several  as- 
pects of  this  behavior  varied  among  developmental  stages 
or  among  types  of  larvae,  including:  the  distribution  of 
particle  captures  among  different  segments  of  the  ciliated 
band,  the  number  of  captures  for  single  particles  en  route 
to  the  mouth,  and  the  frequency  of  particles  lost  after 
initial  capture.  This  variation  raises  a  number  of  questions 
regarding  the  feeding  performance  of  different  larval  spe- 
cies and  the  efficacy  of  these  different  larvae  as  elements 
of  a  reproductive  strategy. 

Introduction 

The  form  and  function  of  suspension-feeding  aquatic 
animals  is  of  wide  interest,  in  part  because  they  face  a 

Received  27  November  1989;  accepted  30  November  1990. 


formidable  challenge:  concentrating  materials  and  energy 
from  a  pool  of  resources  that  is  both  patchily  distributed 
and  highly  dilute.  Many  different  structures  for  concen- 
trating food  from  suspension  have  evolved.  These  struc- 
tures range  from  the  relatively  simple  collar-cell  filters  of 
sponges  to  the  morphologically  and  geometrically  com- 
plex ciliated  gills  of  bivalves  and  setose  appendages  of 
many  crustaceans  (J0rgensen,  1966).  The  method  of  cap- 
turing and  concentrating  food  particles  from  suspension 
undoubtedly  affects  the  effectiveness  of  particle  capture 
and  aspects  of  the  growth  and  metabolism  of  suspension 
feeders  (Conover,  1968).  Inefficient  suspension  feeding 
may  even  limit  the  range  of  alternative  strategies  for 
growth  and  reproduction  (McEdward  and  Strathmann. 
1987).  Thus,  even  if  we  were  not  generally  curious  about 
how  organic  particle  filters  work,  there  are  particular  rea- 
sons (the  diversity  of  filters,  and  the  physiological  and 
evolutionary  consequences  of  this  diversity)  for  investi- 
gating the  nature  of  different  kinds  of  filters  that  remove 
food  particles  from  suspension. 

Suspension  feeding  by  the  planktonic  larvae  of  echi- 
noderms has  been  described  by  a  number  of  authors 
(Gemmill,  1914,  1916;  MacBride,  1914;  Runnstrom, 
1918;  Meeks,  1927;  Tattersall  and  Sheppard,  1934;  Gar- 
stang,  1939;  Strathmann,  1971,  1975;  Strathmann  el  al.. 
1972;  Gilmour.  1985.  1986,  1988a,  b).  These  larvae  de- 
velop a  band  of  tightly  packed  ciliated  columnar  epithelial 
cells  (the  ciliated  band)  that  circumscribes  the  mouth,  di- 
viding the  surface  of  the  larva  into  circumoral  and  aboral 
fields  (see  Strathmann,  1971.  1975).  Early  workers  offered 
divergent  interpretations  of  the  method  by  which  these 
larvae  concentrate  suspended  particles  from  seawater. 
They  variously  attributed  larval  feeding  abilities  to  the 
actions  of:  (/')  cilia  on  the  circumoral  field,  (//(water  cur- 
rents generated  by  the  ciliated  band,  (///)  cilia  surrounding 


12 


FEEDING  BY  ECHINODERM  LARVAE 


13 


N 


0.00 


0.31 


0.51 


0.60 


0.88 


0.94 


1.01 


1.10 


1.16 


Figure  1.  A  collage  of  videotape  frames  showing  the  capture  of  a  20  ^m  diameter  sphere  by  a  six-armed 
echinopluteus  (Dendrasler  e\centricus).  The  number  in  the  upper  right  of  each  panel  is  elapsed  time  in 
seconds  (starting  arbitrarily  at  0  s).  The  arrow  in  the  first  panel  shows  the  initial  position  of  the  particle.  For 
scale,  the  arrow  is  about  135  ^m  long.  The  larva  is  shown  in  anterior  ventral  view,  moving  forward  toward 
the  top  of  each  panel.  The  sphere  moved  toward  the  right  postoral  arm  (0.00-0.51  s).  was  captured  on  the 
ciliated  band  and  changed  direction  toward  the  base  of  the  same  arm  (0.60-0.94  s),  then  was  captured  a 
second  time  near  the  base  of  the  arm  and  moved  toward  the  larval  midline  and  mouth  ( 1.01-1.16  s). 


the  mouth,  and  (;V)  mucus  secreted  between  opposed  parts 
of  the  ciliated  band.  The  more  recent  studies  of  Strath- 
mann  (1971 )  and  Strathmann  et  al.  (1972 (resolved  many 
of  these  conflicting  descriptions:  these  studies  suggest  that 
echinoderm  larvae  remove  particles  from  dilute  suspen- 
sions by  the  brief  reversal  of  the  direction  of  the  beat  of 
cilia  on  the  ciliated  band.  Particles  are  retained  on  the 
circumoral  field,  at  the  upstream  side  of  the  ciliated  band, 
and  then  are  transported  toward  the  larval  mouth.  How- 
ever, Gilmour  (1985,  1986,  1988a,  b)  has  disputed  this 
interpretation  of  larval  feeding  and  has  suggested  two 
completely  different  methods  of  particle  capture. 


Studies  of  suspension  feeding  by  marine  invertebrates 
often  suffer  from  the  inherent  difficulty  of  relating  rates 
of  feeding  to  mechanisms  of  particle  capture.  For  example, 
there  is  no  general  agreement  on  how  the  naplius  larvae 
of  copepods  and  barnacles  capture  particles,  even  though 
these  are  among  the  best-studied  suspension  feeders  (re- 
viewed by  R.  Strathmann,  1987).  The  feeding  mechanism 
of  nauplii  is  difficult  to  study  because  the  movements  of 
the  feeding  appendages  and  food  particles  are  swift  and 
complex.  Measures  of  feeding  rates  of  these  animals  are 
therefore  restricted  to  indirect  observations,  such  as  the 
depletion  of  food  particles  (Paftenhoffer,  197 1 )  or  the  in- 


14 


M.  W.   HART 


Figure  2.  A  cartoon  of  the  particle  capture  sequence  shown  in  Figure 
1.  The  positions  of  the  sphere  in  each  panel  of  Figure  1  are  indicated  by 
the  dots,  and  the  particle  path  between  these  positions  is  interpolated  by 
the  solid  line.  The  ciliated  band  ot  the  larva  is  shown  by  the  heavy  lines; 
the  mouth  is  shown  in  outline. 


corporation  of  radioactivity  from  radiolabelled  com- 
pounds in  food  particles  (Marshall  and  Orr,  1956).  How- 
ever, without  direct  observations  of  feeding,  it  is  difficult 
to  relate  variation  in  feeding  rate  U'.,s,'..  among  different 
naupliar  stages)  to  variation  in  the  morphological  features 
(e.g.,  the  size  and  number  of  setae)  that  determine  the 
feeding  mechanism. 

Unlike  nauplii,  the  feeding  larvae  of  echinoderms  lend 
themselves  to  direct  observation  of  particle  capture.  These 
larvae  are  relatively  transparent,  they  swim  with  slow  and 
continuous  movement,  and  particle  captures  are  suffi- 
ciently slow  events  that  they  can  be  counted  and  described 
with  some  precision.  Given  an  accurate  description  of 
particle  capture  by  these  larvae,  one  can  then  interpret 
quantitative  variation  in  feeding  in  terms  of  the  particle 
capture  mechanism.  Echinoderm  larvae  are  therefore  ex- 
cellent model  organisms  for  comparative  studies  of  form 
and  function  in  suspension  feeding. 

In  this  report,  I  describe  particle  captures  and  suspen- 
sion feeding  by  the  larvae  of  seven  species  from  four  dif- 
ferent echinoderm  classes.  A  qualitative  analysis  of  vid- 
eotape recording  (including  still  video  images  of  particle 
captures)  of  free-swimming  larvae  clearing  a  dilute  sus- 
pension of  particles  is  generally  consistent  with  Strath- 
mann's  description  of  the  ciliary  reversal  suspension  feed- 
ing mechanism.  My  observations  also  refute  Gilmour's 
interpretation  of  the  predominant  method  of  particle 
capture  by  echinoderm  larvae.  However,  a  quantitative 
analysis  of  these  recordings  (which  is  difficult  without  a 
permanent  record  of  larval  behavior)  leads  to  several  novel 
inferences  about  larval  feeding.  First,  these  larvae  appear 
to  have  two  modes  of  particle  capture:  most  particles  are 
caught  by  apparent  ciliary  reversal  at  the  ciliated  band, 
but  a  small  proportion  of  particles  are  captured  without 
contacting  the  peripheral  band,  and  this  proportion  does 
not  vary  among  the  different  larvae  examined.  Second, 
changes  in  the  distribution  of  particle  captures  on  the  cil- 


iated bands  of  larvae  do  not  correspond  to  changes  in  the 
lengths  of  particular  segments  of  the  band  as  larvae  grow; 
some  parts  of  the  band  appear  to  be  more  effective  than 
others,  and  this  discrepancy  changes  during  larval  devel- 
opment. Third,  the  number  of  independent  ciliary  rever- 
sals involved  in  a  single  particle  capture  (from  the  pe- 
ripheral band  to  the  mouth)  varies  among  segments  of 
the  band,  among  developmental  stages,  and  among  species 
of  echinoderms.  These  analyses  also  serve  as  a  basis  for 
quantitative  comparisons  of  feeding  performance  among 
echinoderm  larvae  of  different  size,  shape,  and  develop- 
mental stage  that  will  be  presented  elsewhere. 

Materials  and  Methods 

Colled// in  of  adults 

The  sea  urchin  Strongylocentrotus  pitrpiiratm  (Stimp- 
son,  1857)  (O.  Echinoida)  was  collected  from  tidepools 
at  Botanical  Beach,  Renfrew  County,  British  Columbia. 
Canada.  All  other  adults  were  collected  from  intertidal  or 
shallow  subtidal  locations  off  the  San  Juan  Islands.  San 
Juan  County,  Washington,  USA.  Strongylocentrotus 
droebachiensis  (O.  F.  Miiller,  1776)  and  the  sea  star  Sty- 
lasterias  forreri  (de  Loriol,  1887)  (O.  Forcipulatida)  were 
collected  by  dredge  from  San  Juan  Channel.  The  sea  star 
Dermasterias  imbricata  (Grube,  1857)  (O.  Valvatida)  was 
collected  at  10  m  depth  from  a  rock  wall  off  Turn  Island. 
The  sand  dollar  Dendraster  excentricus  (Eschscholtz, 
1831)  (O.  Clypeasteroida)  was  collected  from  an  intertidal 
bed  in  East  Sound,  Orcas  Island.  The  brittle  star  Ophio- 
pholis  aculeata  (L.,  1767)  (O.  Ophiurida)  was  collected 
from  a  low  intertidal  cobble  beach  in  Mitchell  Bay  on 
San  Juan  Island.  The  sea  cucumber  Parastichopiis  cali- 
fornk'iis  (Stimpson.  1857)  (O.  Aspidochirotida)  was  col- 
lected at  15  m  depth  from  a  silt  bottom  off"  Brown  Island. 

Cull  tire  of  embryos  and  lan'ae 

Gametes,  embryos,  and  larvae  were  treated  according 
to  methods  described  by  M.  Strathmann  ( 1 987).  Gametes 
of  echinoids  were  obtained  by  intracoelomic  injection  of 
0.5  M  KC1.  Asteroid  gonads  were  obtained  by  dissection; 
oocytes  were  induced  to  mature  by  incubation  in  10  6A/1- 
methyladenine  in  seawater.  Parastichopus  gonads  were 
also  obtained  by  dissection:  oocytes  were  matured  in  a  1 
g-r1  solution  oflyophilized  radial  nerve  in  seawater;  and 
sperm  were  activated  in  10  mA/  NH4C1  in  seawater.  The 
radial  nerves  were  obtained  from  the  asteroid  Pycnopodia 
helianthoides  (Brandt,  1835).  Ophiopholis  females,  in 
separate  glass  bowls  filled  with  seawater,  were  allowed  to 
warm  on  the  benchtop  for  several  hours  and  were  thus 
induced  to  spawn;  sperm  were  obtained  by  dissection. 


FEEDING  BY  ECHINODERM  LARVAE 


15 


0.00 


" 


0.52 


0.20 


0.71 


0.57 


0.41 


0.84 


Figure  3.  A  collage  of  videotape  frames  showing  the  capture  of  a  20  /jm  diameter  sphere  by  an  eight- 
armed  ophiopluteus  (Ophiopholis  acn/ea/a)  (the  short  postoral  arms  are  not  visible  in  this  view).  Numbers 
and  arrow  as  in  Figure  1.  For  scale,  the  arrow  is  87  jjm  long.  The  larva  is  shown  in  anterior  ventral  view, 
moving  forward  toward  the  lower  left  of  each  panel.  The  sphere  moved  past  the  tips  of  the  right  anterolateral 
and  posterodorsal  arms  (0.00-0.52  s),  was  captured  on  the  right  posterolateral  arm  (0.57-0.60  s),  and  changed 
direction  back  toward  the  larval  mouth  (0.71-0.94  s). 


0.60 


0.94 


For  all  species,  eggs  were  washed  in  5-^m  filtered  seawater, 
fertilized  with  a  few  drops  of  a  dilute  sperm  suspension, 
then  washed  again  and  transferred  in  groups  of  a  few 
thousand  to  3-1  glass  jars  filled  with  filtered  seawater.  The 
jars  were  immersed  in  a  flowing  seawater  bath  at  tem- 
peratures of  9-l3°C  (near  local  ambient  sea  temperature), 
stirred  gently  by  paddles.  Feeding  larval  stages  were  fed 
2-3  ml  per  jar  from  dense  cultures  of  each  of  three  algae 
(Dunaliella  tertiolecia  Butcher,  Isochrysisgalba.no.  Parke, 
and  Rhodomonas  sp.)  at  intervals  of  five  to  ten  days  coin- 
cident with  water  changes.  These  combinations  of  algae 
produced  initial  algal  concentrations  of  about  10  cells  ^r' 
in  the  jars.  Over  five  to  ten  days,  groups  of  several  hundred 


or  thousand  larvae,  clearing  I -2  yul  '  min  (averaged  over 
time),  probably  captured  most  of  this  food. 

Observing  larval  feeding 

Larvae  selected  at  random  from  the  culture  jar  were 
placed  singly,  by  pipette,  into  the  bottom  of  a  63  ml  cy- 
lindrical glass  observation  chamber  (4.8  cm  diameter  by 
3.5  cm  deep)  containing  a  suspension  of  20  /xm  diameter 
polystyrene  divinylbenzene  microspheres  (Duke  Scien- 
tific) at  a  concentration  of  2.4  n\~ '  in  filtered  seawater. 
The  concentration  of  spheres  was  reduced  to  1  M'~'  for  a 
few  very  large  Dennasterias  larvae  with  very  high  clear- 
ance rates.  In  those  cases  where  the  mouths  of  larvae  were 


16 


M.  W.  HART 


Figure  4.  A  cartoon  of  the  particle  capture  sequence  shown  in  Figure 
3.  The  positions  of  the  sphere  in  each  panel  of  Figure  3  are  indicated  by 
the  dots,  and  the  particle  path  between  these  positions  is  interpolated  by 
the  solid  line.  The  ciliated  band  of  the  larva  is  shown  by  the  heavy  lines; 
the  mouth  is  shown  in  outline. 

too  small  to  ingest  20  ^m  spheres,  or  where  small  larvae 
were  unable  to  capture  these  particles,  10  nm  diameter 
spheres  were  used  (these  cases  include  all  of  the  smaller 
Opliioplwlis  larvae,  and  several  of  the  smallest  Dendraster. 
Parastichopus.  and  Strongylocentrotus  purpiiratus  larvae). 
The  larger  spheres  were  used  whenever  possible,  because 
they  were  easier  to  identify  and  follow  on  videotape.  Lar- 
vae of  a  wide  range  of  sizes  and  developmental  stages 
were  used  for  all  seven  species.  Temperatures  inside  the 
observation  chamber  could  be  held  within  0.5-1.0°C  of 
ambient  seawater  temperature  because  the  chamber  was 
equipped  with  a  circulating  seawater  jacket.  The  top  of 
the  chamber  was  sealed  with  a  clear  plastic  lid,  eliminating 
trapped  air  and  preventing  image  distortion  by  surface 
waves.  As  the  larva  swam  from  the  bottom  to  the  top  of 
the  chamber,  several  minutes  of  feeding  were  observed. 
For  most  larvae,  several  such  feeding  periods  were  ob- 
served. After  each  feeding  period,  the  larva  was  returned 
to  the  bottom  of  the  chamber  by  pipette  and  observed  as 
it  again  swam  upward. 

Some  larvae  did  not  swim  or  capture  spheres  at  high 
rates.  These  individuals  were  not  used  in  subsequent 
analyses.  Slow  swimming,  frequent  stops,  infrequent  par- 
ticle captures,  or  rejection  of  captured  spheres  by  these 
larvae  were  probably  the  result  of  disturbance  during 
transfer  from  the  jar  to  the  observation  chamber.  Under 
the  conditions  described,  many  larvae  swam  rapidly  and 
had  high  clearance  rates,  but  readers  should  not  assume 


that  larvae  exhibit  such  behavior  continuously,  or  that  all 
larvae  will  do  so  under  any  conditions  of  observation. 

I  tried  to  get  larvae  to  capture  and  ingest  a  number  of 
other  kinds  of  artificial  particles,  including  Sephadex 
spheres  of  various  sizes,  other  types  of  plastic  spheres,  and 
ragweed  pollen,  with  variable  success.  I  also  used  various 
unicellular  algae.  Some  of  these  algae  [e.g..  Ixochrysis  gal- 
bana,  Pavlova  lutlwri  (Droop)]  are  small  or  non-refractile; 
others  (e.g.,  Diimiliella  lertiolecta)  are  larger  but  tend  to 
clump  in  suspension.  The  most  promising  cultured  uni- 
cellular organism  was  the  dinoflagellate  Prorocentrum 
micans  Ehrenb.,  which  is  large  and  highly  visible,  does 
not  clump,  and  keeps  itself  suspended  in  water  by  flagellar 
movements.  Unfortunately,  many  larvae  refused  to  cap- 
ture or  ingest  these  cells.  Strathmann  ( 197 1 )  used  the  di- 
noflagellate Amphidiniwn  carteri  Hulburt,  which  I  did 
not  have  in  culture.  Polystyrene  spheres  are  useful  for 
observations  of  suspension  feeding  because  they  are  highly 
retractile,  are  available  in  a  range  of  sizes,  do  not  readily 
form  clumps,  settle  from  suspension  slowly,  and  are 
readily  captured  and  ingested  by  echinoderm  larvae.  An 
added  advantage  of  indigestible  particles  is  that  larvae  are 
unlikely  to  become  quickly  satiated  as  they  clear  particles 
from  suspension. 

Videotape  recordings  of  larvae  feeding  were  made  with 
transmitted  light  at  30  frames  s~'  with  a  videocamera 
mounted  on  the  trinocular  head  of  a  dissecting  microscope. 
I  controlled  both  the  focus  and  field  of  view  manually.  Thir- 
teen to  forty-four  individuals  were  videotaped  for  each  spe- 
cies, and  I  made  some  observations  of  feeding  by  larvae  that 
were  not  taped.  For  illustrations  of  particle  captures,  single 
video  frames  were  captured  from  the  videotape  by  a  frame 
grabber.  The  size  and  contrast  of  the  sphere  were  increased 
in  each  of  these  images,  and  much  of  the  background  con- 
trast was  removed.  These  computer-enhanced  images  were 
then  laser-printed  and  assembled  into  collages. 

I  calculated  a  clearance  rate  (volume  of  water  cleared  of 
particles  per  unit  time,  in  ^1  min"1)  for  each  larva  by 
counting  particle  captures  and  dividing  the  total  number 
of  captures  by  the  length  of  the  observation  period,  then 
dividing  this  capture  rate  (number  min~')  by  the  concen- 
tration of  spheres  in  suspension  (number  ^L~')-  Only  pe- 
riods of  continuous  swimming  and  feeding  were  used, 
therefore  the  calculated  clearance  rates  represent  maximum 
feeding  performance  over  several  minutes.  A  number  of 
laboratory  artifacts,  including  handling  and  transfer,  high 
light  intensity,  and  novel  food  particles,  may  affect  the  rate 
of  feeding  and  the  method  of  particle  capture  (Strathmann, 
1971).  Therefore,  interpretations  of  the  method  of  particle 
capture  must  be  based  on  observations  of  larvae  clearing 
particles  from  suspension  at  near  maximal  rates.  High 
clearance  rates  indicate  that  the  behavior  of  larvae  in  the 
laboratory  has  not  been  strongly  altered  by  any  of  these 


FEEDING  BY  ECHINODERM  LARVAE 


17 


0.00 


0.30 


0.50 


0.66 


0.76 


1.03 


1.20 


1.66 


Figure  5.  A  collage  of  videotape  frames  showing  the  capture  of  a  20  nm  diameter  sphere  by  a  bipinnaria 
(Dermasterias  imbricata).  Numbers  (in  the  upper  left)  and  arrow  as  in  Figure  1.  For  scale,  the  arrow  is  79 
^m  long.  The  larva  is  shown  in  ventral  view,  moving  forward  toward  the  upper  right  of  each  panel.  The 
sphere  approached  the  ciliated  band  on  the  right  side  lateral  to  the  larva!  mouth  (0.00-0.50  s),  was  captured 
there  (0.66  s).  and  changed  direction  back  toward  the  circumoral  field  (0.76-0.90  s).  The  sphere  was  captured 
a  second  time,  on  the  preoral  transverse  ciliated  band  ( 1 .03  s)  and  then  swept  into  the  mouth  ( 1 .20- 1 .66  s). 


artifacts.  Rates  of  growth  and  development  of  larvae  in 
nature  are  probably  often  limited  by  low  phytoplankton 
concentrations  (Paulay  et  al,  1985;  but  see  Olson  and  Ol- 
son, 1989).  High  clearance  rates  are  probably  typical  of 
larvae  feeding  on  these  dilute  phytoplankton  suspensions. 

Measuring  ciliated  band  lengths 

Larvae  were  removed  from  the  observation  chamber, 
killed  in  a  dilute  solution  of  formalin  in  seawater,  then 
mounted  in  a  drop  of  seawater  beneath  a  raised  coverglass. 
Ciliated  band  length  was  estimated  by  summing  the  dis- 
tances between  sequential  landmark  points  on  the  band 


(such  as  the  tips  and  bases  of  the  larval  arms  of  plutei). 
The  planar  location  of  each  landmark  was  determined  by 
digitizing  a  camera  lucida  tracing  of  the  band  for  each 
mounted  larva;  the  location  of  each  landmark  in  the  third 
dimension,  when  in  focus  under  the  microscope,  was  de- 
termined from  the  vertical  displacement  of  the  microscope 
stage  (McEdward,  1985). 


Results 


Particle  captures 


All  larvae  typically  swam  with  the  anterior  end  upper- 
most, from  the  bottom  of  the  observation  chamber,  up 


18 


M.  W.  HART 


Figure  6.  A  cartoon  of  the  particle  capture  sequence  shown  in  Figure 
5.  The  positions  of  the  sphere  in  each  panel  of  Figure  5  are  indicated  by 
the  dots,  and  the  particle  path  between  these  positions  is  interpolated  by 
the  solid  line.  The  ciliated  band  of  the  larva  is  shown  by  the  heavy  lines; 
the  mouth  and  stomach  are  shown  in  outline. 


toward  the  observer  and  videocamera,  capturing  spheres 
as  they  swam.  Runnstrom  (1918)  described  this  and  a 
variety  of  other  swimming  postures;  I  observed  some  of 
them  (most  notably  a  lateral  swimming  direction,  usually 
with  the  ventral  side  uppermost,  as  the  larva  swam  slowly 
along  the  bottom  of  the  chamber).  These  alternative 
swimming  patterns  were  usually  associated  with  low  rates 
of  feeding  and  frequent  general  ciliary  arrests  during  which 
the  larva  came  to  a  halt  on  the  chamber  bottom.  I  am 
not  sure  whether  these  behaviors  are  likely  to  be  common 
in  the  plankton. 

The  aborally  directed  beat  of  cilia  on  the  ciliated  band 
produces  water  currents  with  a  net  posterior  component 
that  drives  the  larva  forward  while  moving  water  laden 
with  particles  toward  the  ciliated  band.  Polystyrene 
spheres  entrained  in  these  currents  approached  the  ciliated 
band  on  the  upstream  side  of  the  band  (usually  on  the 
arms  of  plutei,  or  on  the  loops  of  band  between  the  bases 
of  the  arms,  and  on  the  anterior,  posterior,  and  lateral 
portions  of  the  band  on  bipinnariae  and  auriculariae).  In 
cases  where  the  proximity  of  the  particle  to  the  ciliated 
band  could  be  judged,  spheres  appeared  to  approach 
within  about  one  diameter  of  the  surface  of  the  larva  ( 10- 
20  ^m),  less  than  the  length  of  the  cilia  on  most  parts  of 
the  ciliated  band  (20-30  ^m;  Strathmann,  1971;  Mc- 
Edward,  1984).  For  larvae  that  were  actively  feeding, 
spheres  approached  the  ciliated  band,  then  abruptly 
changed  direction  at  the  band,  and  moved  back  toward 
the  circumoral  field  rather  than  passing  over  the  band 
toward  the  aboral  field.  On  nearby  portions  of  the  band. 


water  continued  to  pass  over  the  band,  while  spheres  were 
retained  on  the  circumoral  field  (thus  they  were  concen- 
trated from  suspension).  Subsequent  to  this  initial  capture, 
spheres  caught  near  the  mouth  often  were  swept  imme- 
diately into  the  suboral  pocket,  probably  aided  by  the 
beat  of  cilia  on  the  circumoral  field  (Runnstrom.  1918) 
and  by  water  currents  generated  by  the  aboral  beat  of  cilia 
on  the  transverse  portions  of  the  ciliated  band  directly 
anterior  and  posterior  to  the  mouth  (the  preoral  and  pos- 
toral  transverse  bands,  respectively;  see  Strathmann, 
197 1 ).  Spheres  captured  at  any  great  distance  (more  than 
50-100  fjm)  anterior  or  posterior  to  the  mouth  were  often 
captured  repeatedly  on  portions  of  the  ciliated  band  pro- 
gressively closer  to  the  mouth;  they  were  then  transported 
to  the  mouth,  probably  by  the  same  two  mechanisms  de- 
scribed above.  I  observed  hundreds  of  such  captures  for 
each  species  examined;  the  specific  descriptions  that  follow 
are  for  four  particular  species  (one  for  each  larval  type), 
but  they  apply  equivalently  to  other  larvae  of  the  same 
type. 

Figures  1.  3,  5,  and  7  show  sequences  of  frames,  from 
videotapes  of  particle  captures  like  those  described  above, 
for  an  echinopluteus  (Dendraster  excentricus.  Fig.  1),  an 
ophiopluteus  (Ophiopholis  aculeata.  Fig.  3),  a  bipinnaria 
(Dcrmasterias  imbricalu.  Fig.  5),  and  an  auricularia 
(Parastichopus  califomicns.  Fig.  7).  The  accompanying 
line  drawings  (Figs.  2,  4,  6,  and  8)  depict  the  paths  of 
spheres  shown  in  the  photocollages.  These  four  pictorial 
accounts  of  particle  captures  are  representative  of  almost 
all  of  the  several  thousand  captures  that  I  observed.  Figures 
1  and  2  show  the  abrupt  change  in  direction  of  a  sphere 
at  the  ciliated  band  of  a  six-armed  echinopluteus,  on  the 
right  postoral  arm  (the  larva  is  shown  in  ventral  view). 
The  sphere  was  captured  twice  enroute  to  the  mouth,  once 
near  the  arm  tip,  and  once  nearer  the  base  of  the  arm.  A 
similar  pluteus  capture,  on  the  right  posterolateral  arm 
of  an  advanced  ophiopluteus,  is  shown  in  Figures  3  and 
4.  In  this  sequence,  the  sphere  was  held  briefly  on  the 
ciliated  band  on  the  leading  edge  of  the  arm,  then  moved 
back  toward  the  circumoral  field  (between  the  opposed 
bands  on  the  arm)  and  the  mouth.  Because  the  oral  hood 
above  the  mouths  of  these  larvae  is  opaque,  the  end  of 
the  particle  path  cannot  be  followed  into  the  mouth  and 
esophagus.  Figures  5  and  6  illustrate  the  capture  of  a 
sphere  by  a  large  bipinnaria:  the  sphere  first  approached 
the  ciliated  band  on  the  right  side  of  the  larva,  lateral  to 
the  suboral  pocket  and  mouth.  The  sphere  crossed  the 
circumoral  field,  was  arrested  at  the  band,  and  moved 
back  toward  the  mouth;  it  was  captured  again  on  the  an- 
terior transverse  ciliated  band  (near  the  mouth)  and  was 
then  swept  into  the  mouth.  A  similar  capture  by  an  au- 
ricularia is  shown  in  Figures  7  and  8;  the  sphere  was  cap- 
tured first  on  the  dorsal  part  of  the  ciliated  band  anterior 


FEEDING  BY  ECHINODERM   LARVAE 


19 


0.00 


0.42 


0.67 


1.69 


Figure  7.  A  collage  of  videotape  frames  showing  the  capture  of  a  20  ^m  diameter  sphere  by  an  auricularia 
(Parastichopus  californicus).  Numbers  and  arrow  as  in  Figure  I.  For  scale,  the  arrow  is  1 28  ^m  long.  The 
larva  is  shown  in  ventral  view,  moving  forward  toward  the  upper  left  ot  each  panel.  The  sphere  approached 
the  dorsal  ciliated  band  on  the  right  side  anterior  to  the  larval  mouth  (0.00-0.42  s),  was  captured  there 
(0.067  s).  and  changed  direction  posteriorly  along  the  circumoral  field  toward  the  right  lateral  portion  of 
the  band  (1.09-1.39  s).  The  sphere  was  captured  a  second  time,  lateral  to  the  mouth  (1. 69  s),  and  then 
moved  toward  the  larval  midline  and  into  the  mouth  ( 1. 83-2. 69  s). 


to  the  mouth,  then  was  recaptured  on  the  lateral  ciliated 
band  before  entering  the  suboral  pocket  and  mouth. 

Larvae  of  all  species  occasionally  captured  spheres 
without  close  approach  of  the  sphere  to  the  ciliated 
band,  and  without  abrupt  change  in  the  direction  of 
movement  of  the  sphere  at  the  band.  Such  a  particle 
capture  (by  the  same  Dennasterias  larva  illustrated  in 
Figs.  5  and  6)  is  shown  in  Figures  9  and  10.  These  few 
spheres  followed  broad,  curving  paths  into  the  suboral 
pocket  of  the  larva,  where  they  were  swept  into  the  larval 
mouth  (probably  by  the  current  generated  by  the  cir- 
cumoral cilia).  These  particle  paths  resembled  those 


described  by  GUmour  (1985,  1986,  1988b).  Strathmann 
(1971)  also  depicted  such  particle  captures,  but  did  not 
emphasize  their  frequency  or  importance.  I  observed 
44  individuals  of  Strongylocentrotus  droebachiensis 
capture  1594  spheres;  of  these,  only  80  (5.2%)  were 
caught  without  an  approach  and  a  change  of  direction 
at  the  ciliated  band.  Similar  proportions  obtained  for 
1 3  Parastichopus  (23  of  438  captures  without  ciliary 
reversals,  (5.3%)  and  17  Dennasterias  (24  of  504  cap- 
tures, 4.8%).  These  proportions  do  not  vary  significantly 
among  species  (compared  by  contingency  table  analysis, 
X2  =  0.118,  P>  0.90). 


20 


M.   W.   HART 


Figure  8.  A  cartoon  of  the  particle  capture  sequence  shown  in  Figure 
7.  The  positions  of  the  sphere  in  each  panel  of  Figure  7  are  indicated  b> 
the  dots,  and  the  particle  path  between  these  positions  is  interpolated  by 
the  solid  line.  The  ciliated  band  of  the  larva  is  shown  by  the  heavy  lines; 
the  mouth  and  stomach  are  shown  in  outline. 


Some  readers  may  be  unconvinced  that  collages  of  still 
video  frames  can  accurately  represent  the  dynamic  events 
involved  in  particle  capture  by  these  echinoderm  larvae. 
I  encourage  such  readers  to  photocopy  the  collages  (en- 
larging them,  if  possible),  to  cut  the  frames  of  each  collage 
out  of  the  photocopy,  and  then  to  view  the  frames,  as  a 
stack  of  flip  pictures,  thus  simulating  the  particle  move- 
ment that  occurs  during  the  capture  of  spheres.  Especially 
skeptical  readers,  who  will  be  persuaded  by  nothing  else, 
can  contact  me  about  receiving  a  copy  of  a  short  videotape 
sequence  that  demonstrates  these  particle  captures. 

The  distribution  oj  particle  captures  on  ciliated  bands 

Spheres  were  caught  on  all  parts  of  the  ciliated  bands 
of  larvae,  including  the  most  anterior  and  posterior  por- 
tions of  the  bands  of  auriculariae  and  bipinnariae  and  the 
tips  of  the  arms  of  echinoplutei  and  ophioplutei.  For 
Parastichopus  larvae,  169  spheres  (41.0%)  were  captured 
by  ciliary  reversal  on  the  anterior  portions  of  the  ciliated 
band,  118  (28.5%)  on  the  band  lateral  to  the  suboral 
pocket  and  mouth,  and  127  (30.7%)  on  the  portions  of 
the  band  posterior  to  the  mouth;  for  Dermasterias  larvae, 
the  same  distribution  was  242  (50.4%)  anterior,  122 
(25.4%)  lateral,  and  1 16  (24.2%)  posterior  captures  (Table 
I).  These  distributions  vary  significantly  between  species 
(compared  by  contingency  table  analysis,  X2  =  8.471,  P 
=  0.015),  perhaps  because  the  lengths  of  the  different  seg- 
ments of  the  band  vary  as  well.  This  is  a  difficult  com- 
parison (between  the  lengths  of  segments  of  the  band  and 
the  proportion  of  captures  by  those  segments)  for  bipin- 
nariae and  auriculariae,  because  the  same  landmarks  that 


can  be  used  to  identify  the  locations  of  captures  on  vid- 
eotape cannot  always  be  precisely  identified  on  the  draw- 
ings of  ciliated  bands  used  to  measure  band  lengths. 

A  similar  comparison  is  more  easily  made  among  dif- 
ferent developmental  stages  of  echinoplutei,  because  such 
landmarks  (the  tips  and  bases  of  the  larval  arms)  are 
readily  identifiable  on  these  larvae  from  all  aspects.  The 
growth  of  early  pluteus  stages  involves  the  addition  of 
ciliated  band  to  only  a  few  portions  of  the  band  (especially 
the  postoral  and  anterolateral  arms),  whereas  larger  plutei 
grow  by  elongating  other  arm  pairs,  as  well  as  that  part 
of  the  band  carried  on  the  body  of  the  larva  (see  Strath- 
mann,  1971,  1975).  All  segments  of  the  ciliated  band  (four 
arm  pairs  and  the  larval  body)  grew  as  Strongylocentrotus 
droehachiensis  larvae  progressed  from  four-  to  six-  to 
eight-armed  stages  (Fig.  1 1 );  most  of  Ihe  post  hoc  pairwise 
contrasts  (four-  v.v.  six-armed,  or  six-  vs.  eight-armed) 
among  these  mean  band  lengths  were  significant  (Table 
II).  But  in  three  cases,  these  size  increases  led  to  no  mea- 
surable increase  in  the  maximum  clearance  rate  of  the 
same  segment  (determined  by  counting  particle  captures 
on  each  segment).  Eight-armed  larvae  had  longer  postoral 
and  anterolateral  arms,  and  longer  ciliated  bands  on  the 
larval  body,  than  did  six-armed  larvae,  but  mean  clearance 
rates  for  these  segments  of  the  ciliated  band  were  no  greater 
for  the  more  advanced  larval  stage  (Fig.  1 1,  Table  II).  In 
a  fourth  case,  feeding  performance  for  one  segment  of  the 
band  declined:  the  length  of  the  ciliated  band  borne  on 
the  larval  body  was  similar  for  four-armed  and  six-armed 
stages,  but  the  mean  clearance  rate  for  that  portion  of  the 
band  was  significantly  lower  for  the  later  larval  stage.  The 
lack  of  correspondence  between  size  and  performance  of 
various  parts  of  the  ciliated  bands  of  plutei  suggests  that 
some  segments  of  the  band  are  more  effective  at  particle 
capture  than  other  segments,  and  that  this  variation 
among  segments  changes  as  larvae  develop. 

Repeated  capture  of  particles 

One  striking  aspect  of  particle  capture  by  echinoderm 
larvae  was  the  repeated  capture  of  individual  spheres  on 
the  ciliated  band.  Figures  1  and  7  show  good  examples 
of  such  events.  In  many  cases,  these  repeated  captures 
produced  a  sort  of  pinball  effect  as  spheres  "bounced" 
from  peripheral  portions  of  the  band  to  segments  of  the 
band  nearer  the  mouth.  I  counted  as  many  as  1 1  distinct 
capture  events  for  single  spheres  caught  by  Dermasterias 
and  Parastichopus  larvae  (Table  I),  though  most  spheres 
were  captured  1-4  times,  and  even  spheres  captured  near 
the  most  anterior  or  posterior  ends  of  the  band  could  be 
transported  directly  to  the  mouth  after  a  single  capture 
on  the  band.  The  mean  number  of  captures  varied  among 
segments  of  the  band  (anterior,  lateral,  and  posterior  to 


FEEDING  BY  ECHINODERM  LARVAE 


21 


0.00 


0.93 


/• 


•>''.-  ' 


••'••  / 

••' 


^     ?''*- 
t,-r 


1.92 


2.32 


2.62 


Figure  9.  A  collage  of  videotape  frames  showing  the  capture  of  a  20  ^m  diameter  sphere  by  a  bipinnaria 
(Dermasterias  imbricala).  Numbers  and  arrow  as  in  Figure  5.  For  scale,  the  arrow  is  89  ^m  long.  The  larva 
is  shown  in  ventral  view,  moving  forward  toward  the  top  of  each  panel.  The  sphere  approached  the  left 
anterior  side  of  the  larva  (0.00-1.74  s)  and  was  swept  directly  into  the  larval  mouth  (1.92-2.62  s)  without 
close  approach  to  any  part  of  the  ciliated  band  and  without  changing  direction  at  the  band. 


the  mouth)  for  both  species.  Spheres  initially  caught  lateral 
to  the  mouth  were  captured  fewer  times  before  ingestion 
than  were  spheres  caught  either  anterior,  or  posterior,  to 
the  mouth  (comparison  of  mean  capture  numbers  by 
analysis  of  variance  and  post  hoc  contrasts  for  Parasti- 
chopiis,  ¥  =  39.60;  for  Dermasterias,  F  =  69.84;  for  both 
comparisons,  P  <  0.001).  Spheres  caught  initially  on  the 
anterior  part  of  the  ciliated  band  were  also  captured  more 
times  than  those  caught  initially  on  the  posterior  end  of 
the  larva  (for  Parastichopus,  F  =  16.96,  P  <  0.001;  for 
Dermasterias,  F  =  5.60;  P  =  0.018).  The  mean  (±  one 
standard  deviation)  number  of  captures  for  all  spheres 
was  also  greater  for  Parastichopus  (2.123  ±  1.254)  than 


for  Dermasterias  (1.944  ±  0.890)  (compared  by  /-test,  / 
=  2.488,  P  =  0.013).  These  observations  support  the 
probable  role  of  cilia  on  the  circumoral  field  in  trans- 
porting captured  particles  to  the  mouth.  Spheres  captured 
several  hundred  micrometers  posterior  to  the  mouth  could 
be  moved  swiftly  to  the  suboral  pocket,  in  spite  of  the 
anterior  direction  of  movement  of  the  whole  larva.  In 
similar  captures,  larvae  of  Parastichopus,  which  lack  cir- 
cumoral ciliation  (Strathmann,  1971),  retained  captured 
spheres  more  often  en  route  to  the  mouth  (see  above) 
than  did  asteroid  larvae,  whicrfjiave  abundant  circumoral 
cilia  (Gemmill,  1914,  1916;  Tattersall  and  Sheppard, 
1934;  Strathmann,  1971). 


22 


M.  W.  HART 


Figure  10.  A  cartoon  of  the  particle  capture  sequence  shown  in  Figure 
9.  The  positions  of  the  sphere  in  each  panel  of  Figure  9  are  indicated  by 
the  dots,  and  the  particle  path  between  these  panels  is  interpolated  by 
the  solid  line.  The  ciliated  band  of  the  larva  is  shown  by  the  heavy  lines; 
the  mouth  and  stomach  are  shown  in  outline. 


Most  spheres  caught  by  echinoplutei  were  captured  just 
once  on  the  ciliated  band,  but  the  incidence  of  multiple 
captures  of  spheres  increased  for  Strongylocentrotus  droe- 
bachiensis  as  these  larvae  developed  more  arms:  for  four- 
armed  larvae  (n  =  9),  10.8  ±  2.7%  (mean  ±  S.E.)  of  spheres 
captured  were  retained  at  more  than  one  location  on  the 
ciliated  band  before  entering  the  mouth;  for  six-armed 
larvae  (n  =  18),  16.1  ±  2.5%;  for  eight-armed  larvae  (n 
=  17).  21.6  ±  2.0%.  Analysis  of  variance  ofarcsine-trans- 
formed  proportions  suggests  that  this  is  a  significant  in- 
crease in  the  incidence  of  multiple  captures  of  spheres  (F 
=  4. 1 1,  P  =  0.023).  Thus  the  complexity  of  particle  paths 
to  the  mouth  increases  as  plutei  increase  in  size  and  change 
shape. 

Retention  of  captured  particles 

Larvae  of  all  species  rarely  failed  to  move  to  the  mouth 
particles  that  had  been  removed  from  suspension  at  the 
ciliated  band.  For  example,  of  443  spheres  captured  by 
Parastichopus  larvae  at  the  ciliated  band  (where  the  site 
and  number  of  captures  for  each  sphere  could  be  deter- 
mined), only  29  (6.5%)  were  lost  before  reaching  the 
mouth  (Table  I);  Dermasterias  larvae  lost  only  11  of  49 1 
such  spheres  (2.2%).  The  frequency  of  loss  did  not  vary 
significantly  among  segments  of  the  band  (anterior,  lateral, 
and  posterior  to  the  mouth)  for  Dermasterias  larvae 
(compared  by  contingency  table  analysis,  \2  =  0.71,  P 
>  0.25).  The  same  proportions  varied  significantly  for 
Parastichopus  (X2  =  12.33,  P  <  0.001),  mainly  because  I 


observed  no  spheres  lost  from  the  lateral  portions  of  the 
ciliated  bands  of  these  larvae.  The  certainty  of  retention 
and  transport  from  the  initial  site  of  capture  to  the  mouth, 
often  a  distance  of  hundreds  of  micrometers,  was  re- 
markable. The  exceptions  to  this  generalization  include 
a  few  small  echinoplutei  and  bipinnariae  that  were  unable 
to  retain  the  larger  spheres  at  the  ciliated  band,  and  some 
ophiuroid  larvae  that  occasionally  captured  spheres  with- 
out ingesting  them.  In  these  cases,  some  spheres  ap- 
proached the  ciliated  band  on  the  upstream  side,  changed 
direction  toward  the  circumoral  field,  then  subsequently 
passes  over  the  band  and  were  lost.  Thus,  under  some 
circumstances,  some  larvae  may  reject  particles  before 
they  reach  the  mouth.  Control  over  particle  captures  at 
the  ciliated  band  may  allow  the  collection  of  food  to  be 
inhibited  even  as  the  larva  continues  to  swim  forward 


Table  I 

Main  niunher  of  captures  lor  single  spheres  caught  hy  larvae  of  (A) 
Parastichopus  caiifornicus  and  (B)  Dermasterias  imbncata 

A.  Parastichopus  caiifornicus 

Ciliated  band  segment 


Anterior 

Lateral 

Posterior 

x  (range) 
SD 

n 

(Spheres  ingested) 

2.592(1-11) 
1.510 
169 

1.517  (1-7) 
0.855 
118 

2.039(1-5) 
0.858 

127 

X  (range) 
SD 
n 

(Spheres  not  ingested) 

2.643(1-5) 
1.277 
14 

0 

2.733(1-10) 
2.314 
15 

B.  Dcnnaslenas  nnhriciiui 

Ciliated  band  segment 


Anterior 


Lateral 


Posterior 


(Spheres  ingested) 


X  (range) 

2.21  1  (1-10) 

1.369(1-5) 

1.991  (1-4) 

SD 

0.947 

0.619 

0.761 

n 

242 

122 

116 

(Spheres  not  ingested) 


X  (range) 

SD 

n 


1.200(1-2) 
0.447 


1.250(1-2} 

0.500 

4 


1 .000  ( I ) 
0 


Observations  are  tabulated  by  ciliated  band  segment  (anterior,  lateral, 
or  posterior  to  the  mouth  of  the  larva)  and  by  capture  success  (ingested 
or  not  ingested).  SD  =  standard  deviation;  n  =  number  of  spheres. 


FEEDING  BY  ECHINODERM  LARVAE 


23 


under  conditions  where  the  mouth  is  jammed  with  par- 
ticles, or  the  particles  are  not  desirable,  or  the  larva  is 
attempting  to  reject  particles  from  its  buccal  cavity 
(Strathmann,  1971). 

Clearance  rates 

Maximum  clearance  rates  ranged  from  1-2  n\  min~' 
for  early  larval  stages  (four-armed  plutei  and  the  simple 
bipinnaria-shaped  larvae  of  asteroids  and  holothuroids) 
with  short  ciliated  bands,  to  6-10  v\  min~'  for  late  larval 
stages  (the  large  eight-armed  plutei  and  the  bipinnariae 
and  auriculariae  with  large  loops  and  folds  of  the  ciliated 
band)  with  longer  bands.  Maximum  clearance  rate  in- 
creases with  the  length  of  the  ciliated  band  in  all  of  these 
larvae  (Strathmann,  1971;  M.  Hart,  unpub.  data). 

These  clearance  rates  are  similar  to  those  of  other  larvae 
of  comparable  size  and  type,  but  measured  by  very  dif- 
ferent techniques.  Strathmann  ( 197 1 )  measured  clearance 
rates  for  larvae  by  two  methods:  counting  algal  cells  en- 
tering the  mouths  of  swimming  larvae,  or  counting  cells 
in  the  guts  of  larvae  left  briefly  in  algal  suspensions.  Lucas 
( 1982)  measured  clearance  rates  for  groups  of  larvae  by 
estimating  the  depletion  of  algal  cells  from  suspension  in 
prolonged  feeding  trials  (of  about  24  h  duration).  The 
similar  range  of  clearance  rates  estimated  for  larvae  of 
similar  types  clearing  algal  cells  or  polystyrene  spheres 
from  suspension  suggests  that  the  use  of  artificial  sus- 
pended particles  can  give  accurate  estimates  of  clearance 
rates.  Flavoring  particles  with  some  transferable  factor 
from  algal  cells  may  enhance  the  rate  of  ingestion  of  poly- 
styrene spheres  (Fenaux  el  al.,  1985),  but  larvae  capturing 
unflavored  spheres,  in  my  study,  ingested  almost  all  of 


Table  II 

pruhahiliiY  value--  /nrposi  hoc  paired  comparisons  «t 
mean  lengths  aiui  <>/  mean  maximum  clearance  rales  among  lan'al 
stages  of  Strongylocentrotus  droebachiensis.  for  di/t'emil  st'xmail* 
of  the  ciliated  hand 


Larval  stage  comparison 


Ciliated  hand  segment 


Four-armed  (9) 
v.v.  six-armed  (18) 


Six-armed  vs. 
eight-armed  (17) 


Postoral  arms 

length 

F  =  34.762"* 

F=  26.461*** 

maximum  clearance  rate 

11.826*** 

3.030M 

Anterolateral  arms 

length 

50.647*** 

38.756*** 

maximum  clearance  rale 

12.149*** 

0.003M 

Posterodorsal  arms 

length 

— 

48.607*** 

maximum  clearance  rate 

— 

6.384* 

Body 

length 

0.9S4"5 

12.290*** 

maximum  clearance  rate 

4.227* 

l.617n! 

Note  that  only  one  set  of  comparisons  for  poslerodorsal  arms  is  made 
(four-armed  larvae  lack  these  arms).  Numbers  in  parentheses  indicate 
sample  sizes  for  each  larval  stage.  ***.  P  <  0.001;  *,  P  <  0.05;  ns,  P 
>  0.05. 


the  spheres  captured  on  the  ciliated  band  (see  below). 
Larvae  may  respond  to  particle  flavor  by  altering  the  rate 
of  forward  swimming  and  water  processing. 

Although  these  different  measuring  techniques  produce 
similar  clearance  rates,  the  techniques  are  not  necessarily 
equivalent.  Strathmann  (1971)  found  that  maximum 
clearance  rates  measured  by  counting  algal  cells  captured 


<u        . 
4 


01 

E 

D) 


468 
number  of  arms 


4         6 
number  of  arms 


D  preoral  arms 

1  posterodorsal  arms 

u  anterolateral  arms 

@  postoral  arms 

•  body 


Figure  11.  A  bar  graph  showing  the  (A)  length  and  (B)  maximum  clearance  rate  of  different  segments 
ot  the  ciliated  bands  (borne  on  the  larval  body  and  on  the  postoral.  anterolateral,  posterodorsal,  and  preoral 
arms)  for  four-,  six-,  and  eight-armed  larvae  of  Strongylocentrotus  droebachiensis.  The  total  height  of  each 
bar  indicates  the  mean  ciliated  band  length  or  mean  maximum  clearance  rate  for  whole  larvae;  for  each 
bar.  the  height  of  segments  with  different  shading  indicates  the  same  measures  for  particular  segments  of 
the  ciliated  band.  Cartoons  of  larvae  above  each  bar  indicate  the  approximate  changes  in  size  and  shape 
from  one  stage  to  the  next. 


24 


M.  W.  HART 


during  periods  of  1-3  min  were  generally  higher  and  less 
variable  than  those  measured  by  counting  algal  cells  in 
the  guts  of  larvae  left  in  algal  suspensions  for  5-13  min, 
presumably  because  the  latter  periods  include  some  in- 
tervals when  larvae  are  not  feeding  rapidly.  Lucas'  ( 1982) 
highest  clearance  rate  for  Acanthaster  larvae  (5.8  ^1  min" ' 
for  early  brachiolaria  larvae)  is  much  lower  than  the  high- 
est maximum  rate  that  I  measured  for  Dermasterias  larvae 
of  similar  stage  ( 10.0  ^1  min"1).  However,  because  of  the 
large  variance  in  clearance  rates  measured  for  different 
individuals,  it  is  difficult  to  make  precise  contrasts  among 
these  three  studies.  Maximum  clearance  rates  measured 
by  watching  larvae  for  a  few  minutes  should  usually  be 
greater  than  rates  measured  by  allowing  larvae  to  feed  for 
many  minutes  or  hours,  but  other  factors  may  obscure 
this  effect. 

One  study  is  not  consistent  with  the  above  prediction. 
Rivkin  et  al.  (1986)  found  exceptionally  high  clearance 
rates  (measured  as  the  incorporation  of  radiolabel)  for 
echinoderm  larvae  capturing  pHlthymidine-labelled 
bacteria.  For  example,  in  feeding  trials  of  ~4  h,  the  mean 
clearance  rate  for  larvae  of  Sterechinus  neumayeri  (an 
echinoid)  was  13.8  ^1  min"1.  The  largest  clearance  rates 
(which  were  time-integrated  averages)  in  their  study  must 
have  been  substantially  higher:  the  mean  +  1  SD  clearance 
rate  for  Sterechinus  was  18.5  ^'  rnin  '.  The  largest  niu.\- 
imum  clearance  rate  I  measured  for  an  echinopluteus  was 
5.4  ^1  min"1  for  a  large  Dendraster  e.xcentricits.  This  is  a 
substantial  difference.  The  thymidine-incorporation 
technique  appears  sound.  Unless  these  Antarctic  larvae 
are  exceptionally  large,  these  clearance  rates  may  reflect 
a  dramatic  adaptation  for  the  rapid  capture  of  very  small 
(<2  jim)  particles.  Measures  of  maximum  clearance  rates 
by  direct  observation  of  these  larvae  would  be  of  consid- 
erable interest. 

Ingestion  of  particles 

Most  larvae  ingested  captured  spheres  by  accumulating 
a  bolus  of  spheres  in  the  middle  and  lower  esophagus. 
They  then  swallowed  the  bolus  into  the  stomach  by  a 
rapid  peristaltic  contraction  accompanied  by  opening  of 
the  cardiac  sphincter.  Most  other  workers  have  observed 
the  same  process.  Other  individuals,  at  times,  did  not 
readily  ingest  spheres,  but  instead  accumulated  them  in 
a  whirling  mass  that  rotated  within  the  buccal  cavity  under 
the  influence  of  water  currents  directed  into  the  mouth 
by  the  adoral  cilia,  and  out  of  the  suboral  pocket  by  the 
transverse  ciliated  bands.  If  this  mass  of  spheres  was  not 
ingested,  it  was  eventually  rejected  from  the  buccal  cavity, 
probably  by  reversal  of  the  direction  of  beat  of  the  adoral 
or  other  cilia  of  the  buccal  cavity  ( MacBride,  1914;  Gem- 
mill,  1914,  1916;Runnstrom,  1918;  Strathmann,  1971), 


and  then  moved  out  of  the  suboral  pocket  over  the  pos- 
toral  transverse  band.  Rejection  of  a  mass  of  spheres  was 
not  accompanied  by  a  general  arrest  or  reversal  of  beat 
of  the  cilia  on  the  ciliated  band  (i.e.,  the  larvae  did  not 
stop  swimming  or  swim  backward),  and  the  rejected  mass 
was  not  captured  again  at  the  postoral  transverse  band. 
These  events  indicate  an  impressive  subtlety  of  control 
over  ciliary  beat  that  is  probably  modulated  by  the  larval 
nervous  system  (Burke,  1978,  1983). 

Discussion 

Methods  of  suspension  feeding  by  echinoderm  larvae 

My  observations  of  particle  capture  by  echinoderm  lar- 
vae suggest  a  resolution  of  the  conflicting  accounts  of  sus- 
pension feeding  by  these  larvae.  The  majority  of  particle 
captures  (by  all  of  the  stages  and  species  of  larvae  that  I 
examined)  were  similar  to  those  described  by  Strathmann 
( 197 1 ).  The  retention  of  particles  on  the  upstream  side  of 
the  ciliated  band  of  larvae,  accompanied  by  a  change  in 
the  direction  of  particle  movement  toward  the  circumoral 
field,  supports  the  hypothesis  that  echinoderm  larvae  re- 
move particles  from  suspension  mainly  by  a  brief,  local- 
ized reversal  in  the  direction  of  beat  of  cilia  on  the  ciliated 
band  (Strathmann  et  al..  1972).  However,  about  5%  of 
all  particle  captures  appeared  to  occur  without  the  close 
approach  of  the  particle  to  the  ciliated  band  and  without 
an  abrupt  change  in  the  direction  of  particle  movement 
at  the  band.  This  proportion  was  similar  among  the  three 
species  1  examined;  larvae  of  a  fourth  species  (Stylasterias 
forreri)  also  captured  about  5%  of  the  particles  that  they 
encountered  when  prevented  from  generating  ciliary  re- 
versals (Hart,  1990).  The  paths  of  particles  caught  by  this 
second  method  were  reminiscent  of  those  described  by 
Gilmour  (1985,  1986,  1988b)  for  echinoplutei  and  bip- 
innariae. 

The  resolution  of  these  conflicting  descriptions  depends 
on  two  factors:  the  availability  of  videotape  as  a  permanent 
record  of  behavior  suitable  for  quantitative  analysis;  and 
high  rates  of  partical  clearance,  indicating  normal  larval 
behavior  uncompromised  by  laboratory  artifacts.  Lacking 
any  permanent  record  of  larval  feeding,  Strathmann 
probably  described  only  the  most  common  mode  of  par- 
ticle capture  that  he  observed  for  free-swimming  larvae 
in  relatively  large  volumes  of  seawater.  For  his  part,  Gil- 
mour has  principally  described  particle  captures  by  larvae 
attached  to  suction  pipettes  or  trapped  between  glass  sur- 
faces, and  such  methods  of  manipulating  and  orienting 
larvae  for  observation  may  disrupt  normal  swimming  and 
feeding  behaviors,  due  to  the  disturbing  effects  of  strong 
suction  by  the  pipette,  or  to  the  close  proximity  of  surfaces 
and  their  large  effect  on  flow  patterns  at  low  Reynolds 
numbers  ( Vogel,  198 1 ).  Larvae  may  respond  to  these  dis- 


FEEDING  BY  ECHINODERM  LARVAE 


turbances  with  reduced  clearance  rates.  At  low  clearance 
rates,  a  few  particles  may  enter  the  mouths  of  echinoderm 
larvae  without  apparent  change  of  direction  at  the  ciliated 
band,  but  this  is  not  the  method  of  particle  capture  that 
is  most  common  when  larvae  are  processing  water  at  high 
rates  (Strathmann,  1971,  1982;  Hart,  1990).  The  particle 
paths  described  by  Gilmour  (1985)  also  occur  in  free- 
swimming  larvae,  but  at  a  lower  frequency  than  his  studies 
suggest.  Because  he  has  not  reported  clearance  rates  in 
any  of  his  studies,  it  is  difficult  to  interpret  Gilmour's 
observations.  Gilmour  has  probably  observed  larvae  that 
are  not  actively  feeding.  To  the  extent  that  larvae  exhibit 
such  behavior  in  nature  (perhaps  in  dense  phytoplankton 
patches,  or  in  response  to  other  disturbances),  these  ob- 
servations may  indicate  the  lower  limit  of  the  capacity  of 
larvae  to  reduce  clearance  rate  in  situations  where  feeding 
is  actively  suppressed.  Gilmour's  methods  are  useful  for 
some  kinds  of  observations,  and  larvae  may  feed  at  high 
rates  under  these  conditions  if  care  is  taken,  but  the  in- 
terpretation of  observations  on  methods  of  suspension 
feeding  made  under  such  conditions  also  requires  careful 
consideration. 

I  cannot  account  for  the  differences  between  Gilmour's 
(1988a)  description  of  particle  capture  by  the  auricularia 
of  Parastichopus  califarnicus  and  my  own  observations 
of  feeding  by  these  larvae.  Parastichopus  larvae  in  my 
study  removed  large  numbers  of  spheres  from  suspension 
in  a  manner  identical  with  that  of  plutei  and  bipinnariae. 
I  could  not  confirm  Gilmour's  (1988a)  observation  that 
an  encounter  between  an  auricularia  and  a  particle  results 
in  a  brief  reversal  in  the  direction  of  rotation  of  the  larva 
and  entry  of  the  particle  to  the  suboral  pocket.  The  ro- 
tation of  these  larvae  was  not  disturbed  by  particle  capture, 
and  they  cleared  spheres  from  suspension  at  rates  com- 
parable to  those  for  other  larvae  of  similar  size  and  de- 
velopmental stage. 

The  kinds  of  descriptions  I  have  presented  are  crucial 
for  the  interpretation  of  quantitative  aspects  of  suspension 
feeding.  For  example,  the  observation  that  echinoderm 
larvae  retain  captured  particles  at  the  ciliated  band  leads 
to  the  prediction  that  the  clearance  rates  of  these  larvae 
should  increase  as  their  ciliated  bands  grow  longer  during 
development  (Strathmann.  1971).  Such  explicit  predic- 
tions are  more  difficult  to  derive  for  larvae  (or  other  sus- 
pension feeders)  where  feeding  rates  cannot  be  determined 
by  direct  observation.  For  echinoderm  larvae,  one  can 
now  try  to  interpret  ontogenetic  and  phylogenetic  varia- 
tion in  feeding  rates  as  a  consequence  of  the  variation 
in  the  length  and  arrangement  of  the  ciliated  band  (see 
below). 

Larval  shapes  and  the  development  of  ciliated  bands 

The  forms  of  echinoderm  larvae  vary  among  classes, 
among  species  within  classes,  and  among  developmental 


stages  of  single  species.  Suspension  feeding  by  these  larvae 
covaries  in  several  ways  with  these  form  differences.  For 
example,  the  number  of  capture  events  for  single  particles 
varied  among  parts  of  the  ciliated  bands  of  both  bipin- 
nariae and  auriculariae,  and  the  same  measure  (averaged 
over  all  segments)  varied  between  these  two  larval  forms. 
The  most  significant  of  these  differences,  I  think,  are  the 
distribution  of  particle  captures  among  segments  of  the 
ciliated  bands  of  echinoplutei  and  the  change  in  this  dis- 
tribution during  larval  development.  For  Strongylocen- 
trotus  droebachiensis.  the  clearance  rate  of  a  single  seg- 
ment of  the  band  was  not  necessarily  reflected  in  the 
growth  of  that  segment  as  the  larva  grows  and  adds  new 
larval  arms.  The  surprising  implication  of  this  result  is 
that  some  ciliated  bands  (on  a  single  larva)  are  more  ef- 
fective suspension-feeding  devices  than  are  other  bands. 
LaBarbera  (1981)  made  a  similar  observation  for  adult 
articulate  brachiopods.  The  ciliated  lophophore  of  these 
animals  consists  of  a  pair  of  lateral  arms  and  a  median 
coil.  The  area-specific  pumping  rate  (which  would  be  pro- 
portional to  a  clearance  rate  if  LaBarbera  had  observed 
particle  captures  instead  of  dye  stream  movement)  of  the 
median  coil  was  only  about  60%  of  the  rate  for  the  lateral 
arms.  LaBarbera  ascribed  this  difference  to  the  geometrical 
arrangement  of  the  different  parts  of  the  lophophore  and 
the  consequences  of  this  geometry  for  shear  stress  and 
viscous  energy  loss  (resulting  in  lower  fluid  flow  rates) 
over  the  median  coil. 

This  inference  (of  shape  effects  on  feeding  performance) 
could  clearly  be  extended  to  variations  on  the  pluteus 
form  among  echinoid  species,  or  to  variation  among  the 
basic  larval  forms  of  different  echinoderm  classes.  Emlet 
(1991)  has  predicted  that  such  effects  could  arise  from 
ontogenetic  changes  in  larval  shape  or  from  phylogenetic 
variation  in  ciliated  band  arrangement.  Using  scaled 
models  of  whole  larvae  with  different  shapes,  or  of  isolated 
ciliated  bands  with  different  orientation,  Emlet  showed 
that  changes  in  both  the  gross  morphology  of  larvae  and 
the  arrangement  of  ciliated  bands  could  enhance  particle 
capture  rates  (by  increasing  velocity  gradients  and  fluid 
flow  rates  over  the  band).  My  direct  measurements  of  the 
feeding  performance  of  different  ciliated  bands  confirm 
that  performance  differences  among  larvae  of  different 
development  stages  do  manifest  themselves,  possibly  due 
to  the  fluid-mechanical  effects  described  by  Emlet.  Other 
observations  (M.  Hart,  unpub.  data)  suggest  that  these 
effects  may  also  extend  to  comparisons  among  different 
types  of  echinoderm  larvae.  If  the  geometrical  develop- 
ment of  a  ciliated  band  affects  the  functional  performance 
of  that  band,  then  there  may  be  taxonomic  biases  in  per- 
formance associated  with  evolutionarily  conserved  differ- 
ences in  patterns  of  larval  development. 


26 


M.  W.   HART 


The  evolution  of  larval  form  ami  reproductive  strategies 

Two  general  conclusions  derive  from  the  previous  dis- 
cussion: all  feeding  echinoderm  larvae  employ  the  same 
mechanisms  to  concentrate  food  particles  from  suspen- 
sion; and  quantitative  aspects  of  feeding  by  these  larvae 
change  during  larval  development.  These  conclusions  in- 
vite some  interesting  corollaries.  First,  the  method  of  par- 
ticle capture  by  echinoderm  larvae  has  remained  similar 
among  different  classes  in  spite  of  considerable  evolution 
of  larval  form.  The  four  types  of  echinoderm  larvae  are 
not  necessarily  related  phylogenetically  in  a  manner  ob- 
vious from  their  gross  organization.  Raff  et  al.  (1988), 
Smiley  ( 1 988),  Smith  ( 1 988),  and  Strathmann  ( 1 988)  have 
all  recently  proposed  phylogenies  for  the  extant  echino- 
derm classes  based  on  different  combinations  of  morpho- 
logical, embryological,  and  molecular  information.  In 
spite  of  the  apparent  similarities  in  elaboration  and  or- 
ganization of  the  ciliated  band  between  ophiuroid  and 
echinoid  larvae,  and  between  holothuroid  and  asteroid 
larvae,  few  of  these  phylogenies  group  the  pairs  of  classes 
together  in  this  way.  There  are  relatively  few  points  of 
agreement  among  the  different  phylogenies  or  among  their 
authors.  One  is  left  to  conclude  that  there  may  have  been 
both  convergent  and  divergent  evolution  of  larval  form 
in  echinoderms.  However,  the  method  of  suspension 
feeding  by  echinoderm  larvae  has  apparently  been  strongly 
conserved  throughout  the  evolutionary  history  of  the 
phylum  (though  numerous  groups  have  lost  the  means 
and  requirement  to  feed  during  larval  development). 

Second,  quantitative  variation  in  feeding  among  echi- 
noderm larvae  may  imply  variation  in  the  effectiveness 
of  these  different  larvae  as  elements  of  a  reproductive 
strategy.  Echinoderm  larvae  (and  other  feeding  larval 
forms)  can  be  thought  of  as  devices  for  turning  small  eggs 
into  large  juveniles  (by  concentrating  materials  and  energy 
from  the  plankton).  The  effectiveness  of  these  devices 
turns  on  the  relative  rates  of  development  and  mortality 
during  larval  life.  The  availability  of  food  to  larvae  affects 
the  development  of  larval  and  juvenile  structures  and  the 
duration  of  the  larval  period  (Fenaux  el  a!..  1985;  Paulay 
et  al..  1985;  Hart  and  Scheibling,  1988).  Larval  duration 
figures  prominently  in  several  theoretical  and  comparative 
treatments  of  life  history  evolution  in  marine  invertebrates 
(Vance,  1973;  Christiansen  and  Fenchel,  1979;  Strath- 
mann, 1985;  Emlet  et  al.,  1987).  Although  all  of  the  larvae 
that  I  have  observed  use  the  same  methods  to  remove 
particles  from  suspension,  they  vary  considerably  in  the 
organization  and  development  of  the  ciliated  band  (see 
Figs.  2,  4,  6,  8).  Some  quantitative  aspects  of  larval  feeding 
vary  as  larvae  change  shape,  or  vary  among  larvae  of  dif- 
ferent classes.  This  variation  may  be  reflected  in  measures 
of  clearance  rates  for  different  larvae.  In  this  case,  we  could 


reject  the  tacit  assumption  that  all  larvae  are  equivalent 
solutions  to  the  problem  of  building  a  large  juvenile  from 
a  small  egg.  The  functional  and  life-historical  conse- 
quences of  such  a  result  are  the  subject  of  a  second  paper. 

Acknowledgments 

The  Director  and  staff  of  the  Friday  Harbor  Labora- 
tories provided  space,  facilities,  and  assistance  for  which 
I  am  grateful.  Larry  McEdward,  Joe  Pawlik,  Richard 
Strathmann,  Malcolm  Telford  and  the  editors  of  the  jour- 
nal provided  encouragement  and  helpful  comments  on 
the  manuscript.  Larry  McEdward  generously  loaned  the 
equipment  and  software  for  measurement  of  ciliated  band 
lengths.  Richard  Strathmann  provided  me  with  his  trans- 
lation of  Runnstrom  ( 1918).  I  was  supported  by  NSF  grant 
OCE  8606850  and  by  an  award  from  the  Graduate  School 
Research  Fund  of  the  University  of  Washington,  both  to 
Richard  Strathmann. 

Literature  Cited 

Burke,  R.  D.  1978.     The  structure  of  the  nervous  system  of  the  pluteus 

larfaofStrongylocentrotuspurpuratus.  Cell  Tisx.  Rex  191:  233-247. 
Burke,  R.  D.  1983.     The  structure  of  the  larval  nervous  system  of  Pisaxler 

nclmiceiix  (Echinodermata:  Asteroidea).  J.  Morphol.  178:  23-35. 
Christiansen,  F.  B.,  and  T.  M.  Fenchel.  1979.     Evolution  of  marine 

invertebrate  reproductive  patterns.  Theor.  Pop.  Biol.  16:  267-282. 
Conover,  R.  J.  1968.     Zooplankton— life  in  a  nutritionally  dilute  en- 
vironment. Am.  '/Mill.  8:  107-1 18. 
Emlet,  R.  B.  1991.     Functional  constraints  on  the  evolution  of  larval 

forms  of  marine  invertebrates:  experimental  and  comparative  evi- 
dence. Am.  /an/  in  press. 
Emlet,     R.     B.,     L.     R.     McEdward,     and     R.     R.     Strathmann. 

1987.     Echinoderm  larval  ecology  viewed  from  the  egg.  Pp.  55-1 36 

in  Eclwwdcrm  Siiulies.  I  '<>/.  2,  M.  Jangoux  and  J.  M.  Lawrence,  eds. 

A.  A.  Balkema,  Rotterdam. 
Fenaux,  L.,  C.  Cellario,  and  M.  Etienne.  1985.     Croissance  de  la  larve 

de  1'oursin  Parucentmtus  lividus.  Afar.  Bid.  86:  151-157. 
Garstang,  \V.  1939.     Spolia  Bermudiana.  I.  On  a  remarkable  new  type 

of  Auncularia  larva  (A.  herinudenxix.  n.  sp.).  Q.  J  Microsc.  Sci..  N. 

S  81:  321-345. 
Gemmill,  J.  F.  19M.     The  development  and  certain  points  in  the  adult 

structure  of  the  starfish  Axlerias  rubens,  L.  Philos.  Trans.  R  Soc 

LoiHl.  B  Bin/  Sci.  205:213-294. 
Gemmill,  J.  F.  1916.     The  larva  of  the  starfish  Porania  pulvillux  (O.  F. 

M.).  Q.  J.  Microst:  Sci..  N.  S.  61:  27-50. 
Gilmour,  T.  H.  J.  1985.     An  analysis  of  videotape  recordings  of  larval 

feeding  in  the  sea  urchin  Lyiechimis  pictus  (Verril).  Can.  J  Zoo/.  63: 

1354-1359. 
Gilmour,  T.  H.  J.  1986.     Streamlines  and  particle  paths  in  the  feeding 

mechanisms  of  larvae  of  the  sea  urchin  Lytt'cliinux  piclux  Verril.  J. 

Exp.  Mm   Bin/.  Kail  95:  27-36. 
Gilmour,  T.  H.  J.  1988a.     Feeding  behaviour  of  holothurian  larvae. 

Am.  /Mill.  28:  I67A. 
Gilmour,  T.  H.  J.  1988b.     Particle  paths  and  streamlines  in  the  feeding 

behaviour  of  echinoderm  larvae.  Pp.  253-258  in  Echinoderm  Biology. 

R.  D.  Burke.  P.  V.  Mladenov.  P.  Lambert,  and  R.  L.  Parsley,  eds. 

A.  A.  Balkema,  Rotterdam. 


FEEDING  BY  ECHINODERM  LARVAE 


Hart,  M.  \V.  1990.     Manipulating  external  Ca:+  inhibits  particle  capture 

b\  planktotrophic  echmoderm  larvae.  Can  J  /no/ .  in  press. 
Harl,  M.  \V.,  and  R.  E.  Scheibling.  1988.     Heat  waves,  baby  booms. 

and  the  destruction  ot'kelp  beds  b\  sea  urchins.  Mar.  Butt  99:  167- 

176. 
Jurnensen,  C.  B.  1966.     Biology  ol'Suspenvon  Feeding.  Pergamon  Press, 

Oxford. 
LaBarbera,  M.  1981.     Water  flow  patterns  in  and  around  three  species 

of  articulate  brachiopods.  J  E.\p.  Mar.  Biol  Ecol.  55:  185-206. 
Lucas,  J.  S.  1982.     Quantitative  studies  of  feeding  and  nutrition  during 

larval  development  of  the  coral  reel  asteroid  Acanthaster planci  (L.), 

J  Exp.  Mar.  Biol  Ecol.  65:  173-193. 
MacBride,  E.  \V.  1914.     Text-book  oj  Embryology  \'ol.  I.  [nvertebrata. 

Macmillan  and  Co..  London. 
Marshall,  S.  M.,  and  A.  P.  Orr.  1956.  On  the  biology  ofCalanusfin- 

marchicus   IX.  Feeding  and  digestion  in  the  young  stages.  J.  Mar 

Biol  Ax.wc.  C  K  35:  587-603. 
Mi-Edward,  L.  R.  1984.     Morphometric  and  metabolic  analyses  of  the 

growth  and  form  of  an  echinopluteus.  J.  E.\p.  Mar.  Biol  Ecol  82: 

259-287. 
McEdward,  I..  R.  1985.     An  apparatus  for  measuring  and  recording 

the  depth  dimension  of  microscopic  organisms.  Trans  Am  Micros 

Soc  104:  194-200. 
McEdward,  L.  R.,  and  R.  R.  Strathmann.  1987.     The  body  plan  of  the 

cyphonautes  larva  of  bryozoans  prevents  high  clearance  rates:  com- 
parison with  the  pluteus  and  a  growth  model.  Biol.  Bull  172:  30- 

45. 
Meeks,  A.  1927.     Bipinnaria  astengera  (Echinodermata).  from  the 

Northumberland  plankton.  Proc.  Zool.  Soc.  Lond .  I'l   1  1927:  159- 

171. 
Olson,  R.  R.,  and  M.  H.  Olson.  1989.     Food  limitation  of  planktotrophic 

marine  invertebrate  larvae:  does  it  control  recruitment  success?. Inn 

Rev.  Ecol.  Sysl.  20:  225-247. 
PafTenhofer,  G.  A.  1971.  Grazing  and  ingestion  rates  of  nauplii,  cope- 

podids.  and  adults  of  the  marine  planktonic  copepod  Calami*  hcl- 

golandicus.  Mar.  Bio/.  11:  286-298. 
Paulay,  G.,  I,.  Boring,  and  R.  R.  Strathmann.  1985.     Food  limited  growth 

and  development  of  larvae:  experiments  with  natural  sea  water.  J 

l-:.\p  Mar  Biol  Ecol.  93:  1-10. 
Raff.  R.  A.,  K.  G.  Field,  M.  T.  Ghiselin,  D.  J.  Lane,  G.  J.  Olsen,  N.  R. 

Pace,  A.  L.  Parks,  B.  A.  Parr,  and  E.  C.  Raff.  1988.     Molecular 

analysis  of  distant  phylogenetic  relationships  in  echinoderms.  Pp. 

29-41  in  Echinodcrni  Phytogeny  and  Evolutionary  Biology.  C.  R.  C. 

Paul  and  A.  B.  Smith,  eds.  Clarendon  Press,  Oxford. 


Rivkin,    R.    B.,    I.    Bosch,    J.    S.    Pearse,    and    E.    J.    Lessard. 

1986.     Bactenovory:  a  novel  feeding  mode  for  asteroid  larvae.  Science 

233:  1311-1314. 
Runnstrbm,  J.  1918.     Zur  Biologic  und  Physiologic  der  Seeigellarve. 

Bergen*  MiiM-iini  Aarhek,  Naturvid  Ruceke.  ,\'r  1 .  60  pp. 
Smiley,  S.  1988.     The  phylogenetic  relationships  of  holothurians:  a 

cladistic  analysis  of  the  extant  echmoderm  classes.  Pp.  69-84  in 

Eclunoderm  Phytogeny  and  Evolutionary  Biology.  C.  R.  C.  Paul  and 

A.  B  Smith,  eds.  Clarendon  Press,  Oxford. 
Smith,  A.  B.  1988.     Fossil  evidence  for  the  relationships  of  extant  echi- 

noderm  classes  and  their  times  of  divergence.  Pp.  85-97  in  Echino- 
dcrni Phytogeny  and  Evolutionary  Biology.  C.  R.  C.  Paul  and  A.  B. 

Smith,  eds.  Clarendon  Press,  Oxford. 
Strathmann,  M.  F.  1987.     Reproduction  and  Development  oj  Mamie 

Invertebrate.*  of  the  Northern  Pacific  Coast.  University  of  Washington 

Press.  Seattle. 
Strathmann,  R.  R.  1971.     The  feeding  behavior  of  planktotrophic  echi- 

noderm  larvae:  mechanisms,  regulation,  and  rates  of  suspension 

feeding./  E.\p.  Mar  Biol  Ecol  6:  109-160. 
Strathmann.  R.  R.  1975.     Larval  feeding  in  echinoderms.  Am.  Zool 

15:  717-730. 
Strathmann,  R.  R.  1982.     Comment  on  Dr.  Gilmour's  views  on  feeding 

by  hcmichordates  and  lophophorates.  Can  J  Zool.  60:  3466-3468. 
Strathmann,  R.  R.  1985.     Feeding  and  nonfeeding  larval  development 

and  life-history  evolution  in  marine  invertebrates.  Annu.  Rev.  Ecol. 

Sysi.  16:  339-361. 
Strathmann,  R.  R.  1987.     Larval  feeding.  Pp.  465-550  in  Reproduction 

of  Marine  Invertebrates  Vol.  V.  A.  C.  Giese.  J.  S.  Pearse,  and  V.  B. 

Pearse.  eds.  Blackwell  Scientific  Publications  and  The  Boxwood  Press, 

Palo  Alto,  CA. 
Strathmann,  R.  R.  1988.     Larvae,  phylogeny.  and  von  Baer's  Law.  Pp. 

53-68  in  Echmoderm  Phylogeny  and  Evolutionary  Biology.  C.  R.  C. 

Paul  and  A.  B.  Smith,  eds.  Clarendon  Press,  Oxford. 
Slrathmann,  R.  R.,  1.  L.  Jahn,  and  J.  R.  C.  Fonseca.  1972.     Suspension 

feeding  by  marine  invertebrate  larvae:  clearance  of  particles  by  ciliated 

bands  of  a  rotifer,  pluteus,  and  trochophore.  Biol.  Butt.  142:  505- 

519. 
Taltersall,  \V.  M.,  and  E.  M.  Sheppard.  1934.     Observations  on  the 

bipinnaria  of  the  asteroid  genus  Luidia  Pp.  35-61  in  James  Johnstone 

Memorial  I  'olume.  LIniversity  of  Liverpool  Press,  Liverpool. 
Vance,  R.  R.  1973.     On  reproductive  strategies  in  marine  benthic  in- 
vertebrates. Am  Sat.  107:  339-352. 
Vogel,  S.  1981.     Life  in  Moving  Fluids.  Willard  Grant  Press,  Boston. 


Reference:  Bioi.  Bull  180:  28-33.  (February,  1991) 


Retarded  and  Mosaic  Phenotype  in  Regenerated  Claw 
Closer  Muscles  of  Juvenile  Lobsters 

C.  K.  GOVIND.  CHRISTINE  GEE,  AND  JOANNE  PEARCE 

Life  Sciences  Division.  Scarborough  Campus,  University  of  Toronto.  1265  Military  Trail. 
Scarborough.  Ontario.  Canada  MIC  1A4 


Abstract.  The  closer  muscle  in  the  paired  claws  of  the 
lobster  Homanis  americanus  become  determined  into 
their  asymmetric  form  of  a  cutter  and  crusher  type  claw 
during  the  4th  and  5th  juvenile  stages  and  differentiate 
their  fiber  composition  accordingly  in  subsequent  juvenile 
stages.  Our  aim  was  to  study  the  effects  of  claw  loss  during 
this  critical  juvenile  period  on  muscle  regeneration.  Hence 
the  fiber  composition  of  the  paired  closer  muscles  in  newly 
regenerated  claws  was  examined  histochemically  following 
removal  of  both  claws  either  in  the  4th  and  5th  stages  or 
in  the  4th  through  7th  stages.  The  newly  regenerated 
muscle  was  retarded  compared  to  its  original  counterpart 
in  both  cases.  In  the  former  case,  however,  the  retardation 
was  temporary  as  the  muscle  composition  in  later  stages 
resembled  the  original.  Recovery  in  the  latter  was  not 
apparent  in  later  stages,  suggesting  that  retardation  is  more 
permanent.  Also  in  both  protocols  the  newly  regenerated 
closer  muscle  occasionally  displayed  a  mosaic  distribution, 
with  slow  fibers  interspersed  among  fast  fibers  in  a  central 
band  that  is  normally  homogenously  fast.  Therefore,  loss 
of  the  paired  claws  during  a  developmentally  sensitive 
period  affects  the  phenotype  of  the  regenerated  muscle 
with  the  change  persisting  for  shorter  or  longer  periods 
depending  on  how  often  the  claws  are  lost. 

Introduction 

Crustaceans  have  an  amazing  ability  of  dropping  an 
entrapped  or  endangered  limb  by  breaking  it  off  at  a  pre- 
formed fracture  plane,  thus  allowing  the  animal  to  escape. 
Because  such  limb  autotomy  involves  little  loss  of  blood, 
the  animal  usually  lives  to  regenerate  a  new  limb.  The 
ability  to  autotomize  a  limb  varies  not  only  among  species, 
or  within  a  species,  but  also  within  an  individual,  in  that 

Received  7  August  1990;  accepted  6  November  1990. 


the  chelipeds  autotomize  more  readily  than  the  walking 
legs.  This  is  the  case  in  lobsters  (Homanis  americanus) 
and  particularly  in  their  juvenile  forms  when  a  gentle 
pinch  to  the  cheliped  will  result  in  autotomy  whereas  the 
walking  limbs  will  need  greater  provocation.  Indeed,  lob- 
sters with  their  solitary  life-style  and  aggressive  nature  of- 
ten lose  claws  in  the  wild  and  often  lose  them  more  than 
once. 

Following  the  loss  of  a  claw,  a  new  one  is  regenerated 
which,  in  structure  and  function,  resembles  its  predeces- 
sor. Although  smaller  in  size  initially,  the  regenerate  limb 
grows  over  several  molt  cycles  to  assume  pristine  pro- 
portion at  which  time  there  is  little  to  distinguish  it  from 
the  original  limb.  A  similar  degree  of  fidelity  applies  in- 
ternally, at  least  with  muscles  that  regenerate  the  same 
fiber  types  as  the  original  in  the  claw  closer  muscles  in 
lobsters  (Kent  el  ai.  1989).  as  well  as  in  snapping  shrimps 
(Govind  el  ai.  1986)  and  crayfish  (Govind  and  Pearce, 
1985). 

A  variation  seen  consistently  in  the  newly  regenerated 
closer  muscle  of  the  claw  in  crayfish  and  occasionally  in 
the  major  claw  of  snapping  shrimps  was  the  appearance 
of  a  central  band  of  fast  fibers  in  a  muscle  that  otherwise 
comprises  100%  slow  fibers.  Because  this  regional  distri- 
bution of  fast  and  slow  fibers  is  reminiscent  of  an  early 
developmental  stage  in  the  closer  muscle  of  crayfish  and 
snapping  shrimps,  it  was  assumed  that  some  aspects  of 
ontogeny  were  recapitulated  during  regeneration.  Such  a 
variation  in  the  phenotype  did  not  persist,  and  the  muscle 
assumed  its  pristine  character  over  the  next  few  molt  cy- 
cles. These  regenerative  events  were  recorded  in  adult 
crayfish  and  shrimps  where  the  muscle  is  fully  differen- 
tiated. What  would  be  the  condition  of  regenerate  muscles 
that  had  not  yet  differentiated  their  adult  phenotype?  We 
studied  this  question  in  the  lobster  Homanis  americanus 


28 


REGENERATED  LOBSTER  MUSCLE 


29 


because  we  were  familiar  with  the  development  of  its  closer 
muscle  (Govind,  1984,  1989). 

The  paired  claws  and  closer  muscles  in  the  lobster, 
Homarus  americamts,  become  determined  into  a  major 
and  minor  type  early  in  juvenile  development  and  sub- 
sequently differentiate  their  claw  morphology  and  muscle 
fiber  composition  into  their  final  form.  While  loss  of  both 
claws  in  the  critical  juvenile  stages  delays  the  determi- 
nation of  claw  asymmetry  to  a  later  stage,  more  prolonged 
loss  suppresses  asymmetry  altogether  (Govind  and  Pearce, 
1989).  With  such  clear-cut  effects  of  claw  loss  on  the  de- 
termination of  asymmetry,  it  seemed  likely  that  muscle 
regeneration  might  also  be  affected.  The  present  experi- 
ments record  the  phenotypic  variations  in  the  paired  claw 
closer  muscles  of  juvenile  lobsters  following  regeneration. 

Materials  and  Methods 

Larval  lobsters  (Homarus  americamts}  were  obtained 
from  the  Massachusetts  State  Lobster  Hatchery  on  Mar- 
tha's Vineyard  and  reared  communally  at  the  Marine 
Biological  Laboratory,  Woods  Hole,  Massachusetts,  by 
methods  described  previously  (Govind  and  Kent,  1982). 
Upon  molting  to  the  first  post-larval  or  4th  stage,  lobsters 
were  reared  individually  in  plastic  trays  containing  pieces 
of  oyster  shells  as  substrate  (Lang,  1975).  On  a  daily  basis, 
the  animals  were  fed  frozen  brine  shrimp  and  checked  for 
molts  to  record  their  juvenile  development. 

Claws  were  removed  by  a  gentle  pinch,  which  elicited 
a  reflex  autotomy,  resulting  in  the  claw  breaking  off  at  a 
preformed  fracture  plane  without  much  loss  of  blood. 
Both  claws  were  so  removed  within  24  h  after  the  animal 
had  molted. 

At  the  appropriate  stages  the  regenerated  paired  claws 
were  autotomized  and  prepared  for  histochemical  ex- 
amination of  their  muscles  based  on  the  stability  of  the 
myofibrillar  ATPase  enzyme  to  the  pH  of  the  incubating 
medium  (Ogonowski  and  Lang,  1979).  Thus,  at  pH  8. 
the  enzyme  is  relatively  stable  in  fast  crustacean  muscle, 
and  hence  these  fibers  stain  more  intensely  in  frozen  cross- 
sections  of  the  claw  compared  to  slow  muscle.  The  his- 
tochemically  treated  cross-sections  of  the  claws  were  pho- 
tographed, and  the  resulting  photographs  were  used  to 
calculate  the  percentage  of  fast  and  slow  fibers.  These  cal- 
culations were  made  from  the  medial  region  of  the  claw, 
which  provides  the  largest  surface  area,  and  hence  is  most 
representative  of  the  entire  muscle. 

Results 

Regenerated  phenotype  is  retarded 

We  have  previously  shown  that  juvenile  4th  and  5th 
stage  lobsters  reared  with  a  substrate  of  oyster  chips  de- 
velop paired  asymmetric  (cutter/crusher)  claws,  while  their 


counterparts  reared  without  a  substrate  develop  paired 
symmetric  (cutter/cutter)  claws  (Lang  el  at..  1978).  Both 
rearing  conditions  were  adopted  for  the  present  experi- 
ments. Thus,  in  the  first  experiment  with  oyster  chips  as 
a  substrate,  the  development  into  asymmetric  cutter  and 
crusher  type  muscles  is  shown  by  plotting  the  percent  of 
fast  fibers  in  the  paired  original  muscles  (Fig.  1A).  One 
of  the  muscles  rapidly  accumulates  fast  fibers  to  make  up 
90%  of  its  mass  and  thus  becomes  a  cutter  type  closer 
muscle.  The  slow  fibers  persist  in  a  small  (10%)  ventral 
band  (Fig.  2b).  The  contralateral  muscle,  which  is  the 
putative  crusher,  shows  a  more  gradual  loss  of  fast  fibers, 
making  up  10-20%  by  the  8th  or  9th  stage  (Fig.  1  A)  and 
becoming  zero  by  the  1 3th  to  20th  stage.  The  fast  muscle 
in  the  putative  crusher  is  restricted  to  a  narrow  central 
region  (Figs.  2a). 

Following  autotomy  of  the  paired  claws  in  the  4th  and 
5th  stages,  the  regenerated  muscles  in  the  6th  stage  have 
a  phenotype  that  is  intermediate  to  the  normal  asymmetric 
condition  (Fig.  1A).  The  fast  muscle  composition  of  the 
paired  regenerated  muscles  is  57%.  and  44%,  while  that  of 
the  paired  original  muscles  is  75%  and  28%.  The  regional 
distribution  of  fast  and  slow  fibers,  however,  is  similar 
between  original  and  regenerated  claws  in  that  the  fast 
muscle  is  restricted  to  a  central  band  while  the  slow  muscle 
appears  on  either  side  (Fig.  2c,  d).  The  regenerated  muscle 
therefore  appears  to  be  retarded  in  its  development.  This 
retardation  is  temporary  because  the  paired  muscles  show 
a  normal  phenotype  by  the  8th  or  9th  stage,  despite  loss 
of  the  paired  claws  in  the  4th  and  5th  stages.  In  other 
words,  recovery  of  the  muscle  phenotype  following  claw 
loss  in  the  4th  and  5th  stages  occurs  within  3  to  4  molts. 
Loss  of  the  paired  claws  for  more  prolonged  periods, 
such  as  from  the  4th  to  the  7th  stage,  successively  results 
in  the  regenerated  muscles  in  the  8th  stage  showing  a  re- 
tarded phenotype  (Fig.  1A).  The  percent  fast  muscle  in 
these  regenerated  muscles  is  52%  and  44%  compared  to 
the  90%  of  a  normal  cutter  muscle.  In  both  retarded  mus- 
cles, the  fast  fibers  are  restricted  to  a  central  band  (Fig. 
3c,  d)  as  compared  to  the  normal  cutter  muscle  in  which 
the  fast  fibers  occur  over  the  entire  area  except  for  a  small 
ventral  band  (Fig.  3b).  The  retardation  in  this  case  appears 
to  be  more  permanent  because  paired  muscles  examined 
in  the  10th  stage  still  showed  subnormal  amounts  of  fast 
fibers,  between  60-70%..  Both  muscles  remained  as  pu- 
tative cutter  types  as  loss  of  the  paired  claws  successively 
from  the  4th  to  the  7th  stage  prevents  the  determination 
of  bilateral  asymmetry  (Govind  and  Pearce,  1989).  Re- 
covery to  80-90%.  fast  fiber  composition  was  still  not  seen 
by  the  13th  to  15th  juvenile  stages,  indicating  that  retar- 
dation of  the  muscle  phenotype  may  be  more  permanent 
in  these  animals. 

In  the  second  experiment  in  which  lobsters  were  reared 
without  a  substrate  of  oyster  chips,  the  development  of 


30 


C.  K.  GOVIND  /.  /  AL 


100 

"   A 

100 

B 

T        I 

80 

I-'' 

-f" 

80 

f 

-  •* 
---'  i 

-•' 

•                         T 

k  '-'"* 

I    ''''J* 

i 

I     -''I 

LU 

0  60 

t     f 

A'              \ 
,                   » 

I 

60 

,>'           1 

''I 

T 

r> 
^ 

T  -''               [ 

A 

T--'     T,-''  i 

i 

* 

i 

CO   40 

T 
•                                 1 

f 

40 

1    X'l 

T 

u_ 

1 

- 

c£ 

i                '  7---- 

.  r 

i 

20 

1                                 | 

1-  -I-.,  i 

20 

- 

- 

i    'i 

- 

n 

i                 I                 I 

i         i         i 

0 

i         i         i 

t 

1                     I 

9  4 

JUVENILE  STAGES 

Figure  1.  Percent  composition  of  fast  fibers  in  the  paired  claw  closer  muscles  of  original  (circles)  and 
regenerated  (triangles)  claws  of  juvenile  lobsters  reared  with  a  substrate  of  oyster  chips  (A)  and  without  a 
graspable  substrate  (B).  For  the  regenerated  condition,  the  paired  claws  were  removed  in  all  of  the  previous 
juvenile  stages.  Each  point  represents  the  mean  and  standard  deviation  of  five  animals.  Curves  fitting  the 
points  for  each  of  the  paired  claw  muscles  is  drawn  by  eye.  The  two  curves  in  ( B)  were  generated  by  arbitrarily- 
assigning  the  muscle  with  the  higher  percentage  of  fast  fibers  to  one  group  (upper  curve)  while  its  counterpart 
was  assigned  to  the  second  group  (lower  curve). 


the  paired  closer  muscles  into  symmetric  cutter  types  was 
followed  by  plotting  the  percent  fast  fibers  in  the  paired 
original  muscles  (Fig.  1  B).  The  paired  muscles  develop  in 
a  parallel  fashion,  accumulating  fast  fibers  until  these 
make  up  80-90%  of  the  total  mass,  and  the  remainder 
are  slow  fibers  restricted  to  a  ventral  band.  In  other  words, 
the  paired  muscles  develop  as  typical  cutter  type  muscles. 
In  comparison,  the  regenerated  phenotype  in  lobsters  that 
had  successively  lost  their  claws  from  the  4th  to  the  8th 
stage,  is  distinctly  retarded  (Fig.  IB).  The  fast  fibers  in 
these  regenerated  muscles  is  between  40-50%  compared 
to  80-90%  in  the  original  muscles.  Moreover,  as  in  the 
first  experiment,  the  retarded  condition  persists  for  several 
subsequent  stages  at  least  until  the  1 3th  stage,  which  is  as 
far  as  we  proceeded  in  this  experiment. 

Regenerated  phenotype  shows  mosaic  pattern 

As  described  above,  the  distribution  of  fast  fibers  in  the 
paired  closer  muscles  is  restricted  to  a  distinct  central 
band.  In  the  putative  cutter  muscle,  this  fast  band  during 
juvenile  development  rapidly  enlarges  to  occupy  almost 
the  entire  cross-sectional  face,  except  for  a  slim  ventral 
band  (Figs.  2b,  3b).  In  the  putative  crusher,  on  the  other 
hand,  this  central  fast  band  gradually  diminishes  in  size 
until  it  completely  disappears  (Figs.  2a,  3a).  Throughout 
these  developmental  changes,  the  central  band  of  fast  fi- 
bers is  homogenous  and  sharply  delineated  from  the  ad- 
jacent slow  fibers. 

The  homogeneity  of  the  fast  band,  however,  was  dis- 
rupted to  various  degrees  in  some  of  the  regenerated  mus- 


cles following  autotomy  of  the  paired  claws.  The  least 
disruptive  case  was  where  slow  fibers  were  occasionally 
interspersed  among  the  fast  fibers,  especially  along  the 
lateral  edges  of  the  fast  band  (Fig.  2c,  d;  3c,  d).  This  gave 
the  fast  band  a  ragged  edge,  which  was  in  contrast  to  its 
usual  sharp  edge.  Much  more  disruptive  cases  involved 
considerable  interspersing  of  slow  fibers  in  the  fast  band 
(Fig.  4a,  b),  resulting  in  a  distinct  mosaic  pattern. 

Discussion 

In  a  previous  study  (Kent  el  ai.  1989),  we  examined 
the  phenotype  of  the  regenerated  closer  muscle  following 
claw  loss  in  late  juveniles  and  adults,  when  the  claws  and 
closer  muscles  were  well  differentiated  into  cutter  and 
crusher  types.  In  these  cases,  the  regenerated  claws  and 
closer  muscles  resembled  their  predecessors  with  consid- 
erable fidelity.  The  present  report  examines  the  effect  on 
the  regenerate  muscle  phenotype  following  the  loss  of  both 
claws  in  early  juvenile  stages  when  claw  type  is  being  de- 
termined (Emmel,  1908;  Lang  el  a/.,  1978)  and  fiber  typ- 
ing in  the  closer  muscle  is  being  expressed  (Govind  and 
Lang.  1978;  Ogonowski  el  til..  1980).  Thus  removal  of 
paired  claws  successively  either  in  the  4th  and  5th  stages 
or  in  the  4th  through  7th  stages  resulted  in  a  regenerated 
phenotype  that  resembled  the  undifferentiated  condition 
in  the  normal  4th  stage  lobster.  In  the  case  where  claw 
loss  encompassed  only  the  4th  and  5th  stages,  the  regen- 
erate muscle  completes  its  differentiation  into  crusher  and 
cutter  types  in  subsequent  stages.  In  the  animals  subjected 
to  more  prolonged  claw  loss  (i.e.,  from  the  4th  to  the  7th 


REGENERATED  LOBSTER  MUSCLE 


31 


Figure  2.  Cross-sections  through  the  paired  original  (a,  hi  claws  of 
a  juvenile  6th  stage  lobster  and  through  the  paired  regenerated  (c.  d) 
claws  of  another  6th  stage  lobster  in  which  the  claws  had  been  removed 
in  the  4th  and  5th  stages.  Histochemical  detection  of  myofibrillar  ATPase 
activity  shows  fast  fibers  staining  more  intensely  than  slow,  and  hence 
the  small,  dorsally  located  opener  muscle  (arrow)  is  entirely  slow  while 
the  large  closer  muscle  occupying  most  of  the  cross-sectional  area  has  a 
central  band  of  fast  fibers  sandwiched  dorsally  and  ventrally  by  slow 
fibers.  The  fast  band  varies  considerably  in  size  between  the  paired  original 
muscles  being  narrow  in  the  putative  crusher  muscle  (a)  and  broad  in 
the  putative  cutter  muscle  (b).  In  the  paired  regenerate  muscles,  however, 
the  fast  band  is  similar  in  size.  Magnification  25X. 


stage),  however,  the  regenerate  muscles  have  not  com- 
pletely differentiated  into  cutter  types  in  the  subsequent 
3-5  stages.  Thus  the  absence  of  the  muscle  during  the 
critical  juvenile  stages  results  in  regenerate  phenotype 
being  retarded.  How  long  the  muscle  is  retarded  appears 
to  depend  on  how  often  the  claws  are  lost;  when  lost  for 
two  successive  stages,  the  retardation  is  temporary  but 
when  lost  over  several  successive  stages,  the  retardation 
is  more  permanent. 

A  few  of  the  regenerate  muscles  had  slow  fibers  inter- 
spersed in  the  fast  muscle  band,  giving  rise  to  a  mosaic 
distribution  of  these  two  types  of  fibers.  This  is  an  unusual 
distribution  of  fast  and  slow  fibers  in  the  closer  muscle  of 
lobsters  as  well  as  other  decapod  crustaceans.  Thus,  in 
the  claw  closer  muscle  of  lobsters  (Ogonowski  el  ill.,  1980), 
crayfish  (Govind  and  Pearce,  1985),  snapping  shrimps 
(O'Connor  el  til.  1984),  and  hermit  crabs  (Stephens  el 


al.  1984),  fast  and  slow  muscle  is  regionally  distributed; 
the  fast  fibers  are  restricted  to  a  band  in  the  central  region. 
The  closer  muscle  in  the  more  anterior  walking  limbs  in 
lobsters  (Mearow  and  Govind,  1986)  and  hermit  crabs 
(Stephens  el  ill.  1984)  have  a  similar  pattern.  In  no  in- 
stance has  a  mosaic  distribution  of  fast  and  slow  fibers  in 
the  closer  muscle  been  reported  in  the  above  mentioned 
species. 

Apart  from  the  closer  muscles  listed  above  containing 
discrete  populations  of  fast  and  slow  fibers,  other  muscles 
that  have  been  examined  are  composed  of  a  single  fiber 
type,  e.g.,  the  abdominal  extensor  and  flexor  systems  that 
have  separate  fast  and  slow  muscles  in  tailed  crustaceans 
(Govind  and  Atwood,  1982).  Consequently,  the  appear- 
ance of  a  mosaic  distribution  of  fiber  types  is  an  uncom- 
mon finding  among  decapod  crustaceans.  That  such  a 
mosaic  pattern  occurs  only  in  regenerated  closer  muscles 
and  not  in  the  originals  suggests  that  the  instructions  for 
differentiating  an  entire  muscle  are  not  as  robust  as  those 


Figure  3.  Cross-sections  through  the  paired  original  (a,  b)  claws  of 
a  juvenile  8th  stage  lobster  and  through  the  paired  regenerated  (c,  d) 
claws  of  another  8th  stage  lobster  in  which  the  claws  had  been  removed 
in  the  4th,  5th,  6th,  and  7th  stages.  The  proportion  of  fast  fibers  is  highly 
asymmetric  in  the  paired  original  closer  muscles  being  restricted  to  a 
narrow  central  band  in  the  crusher  claw  (a)  but  widespread  in  the  cutter 
claw  (b).  In  the  paired  regenerate  muscles,  however,  the  band  of  fast 
fibers  is  symmetric.  Magnification  15X. 


32 


C.  K.  GOVIND  ET  AL 


Figure  4.  Cross-sections  through  the  claws  of  two  juvenile  8th  stage  lobsters  (a.  h)  showing  different 
degrees  of  interspersion  of  slow  fibers  (light  staining)  in  the  hand  of  fast  fibers  (dark-staining),  resulting  in  a 
mosaic  appearance  in  the  closer  muscle.  Magnification  35x. 


for  differentiating  individual  fiber  types.  Perhaps  along 
similar  lines  is  our  observation  that  in  the  regenerated 
chelipeds  of  adult  lobsters,  the  main  limb  nerve  will  often 
travel  in  a  scattered,  diffuse  fashion  rather  than  in  discrete 
bundles.  Although  haphazard  in  appearance,  the  regen- 
erated nerve  contains  the  requisite  motor  and  sensory 
neurons. 

While  a  mosaic  distribution  of  fiber  types  within  a 
muscle  occurs  rarely  in  crustaceans,  it  is  commonplace 
among  vertebrates  where  individual  limb  muscles  are  in- 
nervated by  a  large  number  of  motor  neurons  (Burke, 
1981).  Despite  being  randomly  distributed  within  the 
muscle,  fibers  comprising  a  motor  unit  are  of  the  same 
type.  This  has  led  to  the  suggestion  that  the  innervating 
neuron  regulates  muscle  fiber  properties.  Such  neuro- 
trophic  regulation  in  the  lobster  claw  closer  muscle  is  un- 
likely as  there  are  only  two  excitor  neurons  (Wiersma, 
1961),  both  of  which  distribute  to  most  of  the  muscle 
fibers  (Govind  and  Lang,  1974). 

Our  findings  also  underscore  the  very  robust  nature  of 
the  regenerative  capacity  among  juvenile  lobsters.  Apart 
from  the  slowing  down  in  muscle  differentiation  and  the 
occasional  appearance  of  a  mosaic  distribution  of  fiber 
types,  conditions  that  may  be  ameliorated,  the  regenerate 
muscle  otherwise  resembles  its  original  counterpart.  Thus, 
the  loss  of  claws  seen  particularly  in  the  early  juvenile 
stages  does  not  appear,  in  the  long  term,  to  impede  the 
differentiation  of  a  typical  phenotype  in  the  closer  muscle. 


Acknowledgments 

We  thank  Michael  Syslow  and  Kevin  Johnson  for  gen- 
erous supplies  of  larval  lobsters  and  the  Natural  Sciences 
and  Engineering  Research  Council  of  Canada  for  financial 
support. 

Literature  Cited 

Burke,  R.  K.  1981.  Motor  units:  anatomy,  physiology  and  functional 
units.  Pp.  345-422  in  Handbook  oj  Physiology:  The  Nen'ous  System. 
Vol.  II.  J.  M.  Brookhart  and  V.  B.  Mountcastle,  eds.  Williams  and 
Wilkens.  Baltimore. 

Kmmel,  V.  E.  1908.  The  experimental  control  of  asymmetry  at  different 
stages  in  the  development  of  the  lobster.  J.  Exp.  Zoo/-  5:  471-484. 

Govind,  C.  K.  1984.  Development  of  asymmetry  in  the  neuromuscular 
system  of  lobster  claws.  Bwl  Bull.  167:  94-1 19. 

Govind,  C.  K.  1989.  Asymmetry  in  lobster  claws.  Am.  Sci.  77:  468- 
474. 

Govind,  C.  K.,  and  H.  L.  Atwood.  1982.  Organization  of  the  neuro- 
muscular system.  Pp.  63-103  in  The  Biology  of  Crustacea  I  '<>/.  3.. 
Neurobiology:  Structure  and  Function,  D.  E.  Bliss,  H.  L.  Atwood, 
and  D.  C.  Sandeman.  eds.  Academic  Press.  New  York. 

Govind.  C.  K.,  and  K.  S.  Kent.  1982.  Transformation  of  fast  fibres  to 
slow  presented  by  lack  of  activity  in  developing  lobster  muscle.  Nature 
298:  755-757. 

Govind,  C.  K.,  and  F.  Lang.  1974.  Neuromuscular  analysis  of  closing 
in  the  dimorphic  claws  of  the  lobster.  Homams  americanus.  J  Exp. 
Zoo/.  190:  281-288. 

Govind,  C.  K.,  and  F.  Lang.  1978.  Development  of  the  dimorphic  claw 
closer  muscles  of  the  lobster  llomarus  americanus.  III.  Transfor- 
mation to  dimorphic  muscles  in  juveniles.  Biol.  Bull  154:  55-67. 


REGENERATED  LOBSTER  MUSCLE 


33 


Govind.  C.  K.,  and  J.  Pearce.  1985.     Enhanced  reappearance  of  fast 

fibers  in  regenerating  crayfish  claw  closer  muscle.  Dc\:  Biol   107: 

206-212. 
Govind,  C.  K.,  and  J.  Pearce.  1989.     Critical  period  for  determining 

claw  asymmetry  in  juvenile  lobsters.  J.  Exp.  Znol  249:  31-35. 
Govind.  C.  K.,  K.  M.  Mearow,  and  A.  \\ong.  1986.     Regeneration  of 

fiber  types  in  paired  asymmetric  closer  muscle  of  the  snapping  shrimp, 

Alpheus  heierochelis.  J  Exp.  Biol.  123:  55-71. 
Kent,  K.  S.,  J.  Pearce,  C.  Gee,  and  C.  K.  Govind.  1989.     Regenerative 

fidelity  in  the  paired  claw  closer  muscles  of  lobsters.  Can.  J.  Zoo/. 

67:  1573-1577. 

Lang,  F.  1975.  A  simple  culture  system  for  juvenile  lobsters.  At/iiu- 
ciiltnre  6:  389-343. 

Lang,  F.,  C.  K.  Govind.  and  \V.  J.  Costello.  1978.  Experimental  trans- 
formation of  fiber  properties  in  lobster  muscle.  Science  201:  1037- 
1039. 

Mearow,  K.  M.,  and  C.  K.  Govind.  1986.     Neuromuscular  properties 


in  the  serially  homologous  lobster  limbs.  J.  Exp   Zoo/.  239:  197- 

204. 
O'Connor,  K.,  P.  J.  Stephens,  and  J.  M.  Leferovich.  1982.     Regional 

distribution  of  muscle  fiber  types  in  the  asymmetric  claws  of  Cali- 

fornian  snapping  shnmp.  Biol.  Bull.  163:  329-336. 
Ogonowski,  M.  M.,  and  F.  Lang.  1979.     Histochemical  evidence  for 

enzyme  differences  in  crustacean  fast  and  slow  muscle.  J.  Exp.  Zoo/. 

207:  143-151. 
Ogonowski,  M.  M.,  F.  Lang,  and  C.  K.  Govind.  1980.     Histochemistry 

of  lobster  claw  closer  muscles  during  development.  /  Exp.  /.ool. 

213:  359-367. 

Stephens,  P.  J.,  L.  M.  Lofton,  and  P.  klainer.  1984.     The  dimorphic- 
claws  of  the  hermit  crab,  Pagiimx  pollicaris:  properties  of  the  closer 

muscle.  Biol.  Bull  167:  713-721. 
Wiersma,  C.  A.  G.  1961.     The  neuromuscular  system.  Pp.  191-240  in 

The  Physiology  ol 'Crustacea.  Vol.  2,  T.  H.  Waterman,  ed.  Academic 

Press,  New  York. 


Reference:  Bio/.  Bull.  180:  34-55.  (February.  1991) 


Gastropod  Egg  Capsules  and  Their  Contents  From 
Deep-Sea  Hydrothermal  Vent  Environments 

R.  G.  GUSTAFSON,  D.  T.  J.  LITTLEWOOD,  AND  R.  A.  LUTZ 

Institute  of  Marine  and  Coastal  Sciences,  Rutgers  University,  New  Brunswick,  New  Jersey  08903 


Abstract.  Egg  capsules  from  three  different  prosobranch 
gastropods  were  retrieved  from  the  Galapagos  Rift  and 
Juan  de  Fuca  Ridge  deep-sea  hydrothermal  vent  fields. 
The  morphology  of  these  capsules  and  their  excapsulated 
embryos  and  larvae  are  described  and  illustrated.  Based 
on  their  capsule  type  and  the  protoconch  morphology  of 
their  contained  larvae,  29  lenticular  capsules  from  the 
Galapagos  Rift  could  be  attributed  to  a  provisionally  de- 
scribed neogastropod  turrid,  Phymorhynchus  sp.  But  3 
inflated,  triangular  capsules  from  the  Galapagos  Rift,  and 
56  different  egg  capsules  from  the  Juan  de  Fuca  Ridge, 
each  shaped  like  an  inflated  pouch,  could  not  be  unam- 
biguously assigned  to  a  member  of  the  known  vent  gas- 
tropod fauna.  The  mode  of  development  and  potential 
for  dispersal  is  inferred  from  egg  capsule  type,  the  number 
of  embryos  per  capsule,  and  protoconch  characters  com- 
parable to  those  of  confamilial  shallow-water  gastropods 
for  which  the  type  of  development  is  known.  These  criteria 
and  a  comparison  to  the  known  juvenile  shell  morphology 
of  Phymorhynchus  sp.,  suggest  that,  after  encapsulation, 
this  species  develops  planktotrophically  and  is  capable  of 
long-range  dispersal.  Similar  evidence  suggests  that  the 
larvae  contained  in  the  inflated  triangular  capsules  from 
the  Galapagos  Rift  may  also  develop  planktotrophically 
after  hatching;  but  the  larvae  in  the  pouch-like  egg  capsules 
from  the  Juan  de  Fuca  Ridge  probably  develop  non- 
planktotrophically  without  a  dispersal  stage.  These  de- 
velopmental patterns  are  characteristic  of  shallow-water 
members  of  the  systematic  groups  to  which  these  species 
belong,  indicating,  as  previous  studies  have  shown,  that 
vent  gastropods  can  persist  in  these  patchy,  ephemeral 
environments  in  the  absence  of  unique  adaptations  al- 
lowing dispersal  between  active  hydrothermal  sites. 


Received  14  August  1990;  accepted  30  November  1990. 


Introduction 

Active  hydrothermal  vent  systems  accompanied  by 
dense  benthic  fauna  occur  at  several  widely  separated  sites 
along  active  oceanic  ridges  in  the  eastern  Pacific,  from 
48°N  along  the  Juan  de  Fuca  Ridge,  to  22°S  along  the 
East  Pacific  Rise.  Known  hydrothermal  fields  on  the  Juan 
de  Fuca  Ridge  are  separated  by  as  much  as  100  km, 
whereas  separation  along  transform  faults  on  the  East  Pa- 
cific Rise  indicates  that  vent  fields  are  at  least  100  km 
apart  in  this  region  (Crane,  1985;  Grassle,  1986).  Local 
vent  habitats  appear  to  be  transient,  with  populations 
being  susceptible  to  intermittent  establishment  and  ex- 
tinction (Lulzeial.,  1985;  J.  F.  Grassle.  1985;  Lutz,  1988). 
Despite  their  apparent  geographic  isolation  and  ephemeral 
nature,  vent  areas  are  characterized  by  the  remarkable 
similarity  of  their  faunal  assemblages  (Lutz,  1988).  Fun- 
damental biological  questions  remain  regarding  both  the 
manner  in  which  these  ephemeral  habitats  are  colonized, 
and  the  mechanisms  of  organism  dispersal  and  rates  of 
gene  flow  between  discrete  areas  of  hydrothermal  activity 
associated  with  contiguous  and  non-contiguous  oceanic 
ridge  systems. 

Because  laboratory  culture  of  deep-sea  organisms  is  dif- 
ficult (Turner  et  ui,  1985),  many  of  our  perceptions  about 
the  development  and  larval  dispersal  of  vent  biota  have 
been,  by  necessity,  inferred  from  analyses  of  egg  size,  fe- 
cundity, and  morphology  of  larval  structures  retained  on 
juvenile  and  adult  specimens.  Gastropod  mollusks  have 
been  widely  used  for  such  studies,  because  a  record  of  the 
larval  developmental  pattern  can  be  inferred  from  the 
morphology  of  the  initial  shell,  comprising  the  Protoconch 

I  in  non-planktotrophic  species  and,  also,  the  Protoconch 

II  shell  stages,  in  planktotrophic  species  (Powell,  1942; 
Thorson,  1950;Shuto,  1974;  Robertson,  1976:  Jablonski 
and  Lutz,  1980). 

The  mode  of  larval  development  in  recent  (Rodriguez 
Babio  and  Thiriot-Quievreux,  1974;  Bandel,  1975a,  b,  c, 


34 


HYDROTHERMAL  VENT  EGG  CAPSULES 


1982;  Bouchet,  1976a,  b;  Scheltema.  1978;  Bouchet  and 
Waren,  1979b;  Rex  and  Waren.  1982;  Scheltema  and 
Williams.  1983;  Lutz  ctal.,  1984,  1 986;  Turner  and  Lutz, 
1984;  Turner  ct  al..  1985;Colman  ctal.,  1986;  Lutz,  1988; 
Lima  and  Lutz,  1990)  and  fossil  (Jung,  1975;  Scheltema, 
1978,  1981;  Jablonski  and  Lutz,  1980,  1983;  Bouchet, 
1981;Hansen.  1982,  1983;  Jablonski,  1986)prosobranch 
gastropods  has  been  classified  as  either  planktotrophic  or 
non-planktotrophic  based  on  criteria  of  larval  shell  mor- 
phology formulated  by  Kesteven  (1912).  Dall  (1924), 
Powell  (1942),  Thorson  (1950),  Robertson  ( 1971,  1976), 
Rodriguez  Babio  and  Thiriot-Quievreux  (1974),  Shuto 
(1974),  Sohl  (1977),  and  Jablonski  and  Lutz  (1980,  1983). 

Prosobranch  species  with  larval  shells  having  1.5  to  9 
whorls,  a  distinct  fine  sculpture,  a  brown  coloration  in 
contrast  to  a  white  or  gray  adult  shell,  a  narrow  high  spire, 
a  clear  difference  between  the  Protoconch  I  and  Proto- 
conch  II,  and  possibly  a  projection  on  the  outer  lip  of  the 
larval  shell  which  interdigitates  with  the  velum  [("sinu- 
sigera"  larvae  in  terminology  of  Robertson  (1976)]  are 
categorized  as  planktotrophic.  Species  with  larval  shells 
having  0.5  to  1.5  whorls,  simple  or  no  ornamentation, 
the  same  coloration  as  the  teleoconch,  a  large  bulbous 
apex,  and  no  evidence  of  separation  between  the  Proto- 
conch I  and  Protoconch  II  are  categorized  as  non-plank- 
totrophic. In  the  general  terminology  of  Thorson's  (1950) 
"apex  theory."  shells  of  the  planktotrophic  type  are  termed 
multispiral  or  polygyrate  and  shells  of  the  non-plankto- 
trophic type  are  termed  paucispiral. 

Although  these  criteria  allow  differentiation  between 
planktotrophic  and  non-planktotrophic  larvae,  recent 
culturing  of  trochoidean  archeogastropods  demonstrates 
that  the  presence  of  a  paucispiral  protoconch  is  insufficient 
evidence  on  which  to  discriminate  between  a  planktonic 
and  a  non-planktonic  larval  existence  (Hadfield  and 
Strathmann,  1990).  Of  four  trochoideans  cultured,  Had- 
field and  Strathmann  (1990)  found  two  with  pelagic  de- 
velopment of  7  d  or  more  and  two  with  entirely  benthic 
life  histories,  although  all  four  produced  veliger  larvae 
and  had  similar  inflated  paucispiral  protoconchs.  Al- 
though the  mode  of  larval  development  in  shelled  opis- 
thobranchs  may  also  be  reflected  in  the  larval  shell  mor- 
phology, this  relationship  has  not  been  demonstrated 
throughout  the  group  (Rex  and  Waren,  1982). 

Ockelmann  (1965)  formulated  criteria  distinguishing 
between  planktotrophic  and  non-planktotrophic  devel- 
opment in  a  wide  range  of  bivalves  based  on  relatively 
precise  dimensions  of  the  prodissoconch  I  and  II,  but  the 
only  effort  to  establish  similar  criteria  for  gastropod  pro- 
toconchs was  based  on  data  from  comparatively  few  spe- 
cies (Lima  and  Lutz,  1990).  Nevertheless,  Shuto  (1974) 
has  shown  that,  given  a  complete  Protoconch  I  and  II, 
the  ratio  of  the  maximum  diameter  (D;  in  mm)  of  the 
whole  protoconch  to  the  number  of  whorls  or  volutions 


(Vol)  provides  an  index  to  the  developmental  type  of  a 
marine  prosobranch  gastropod.  A  species  with  more  than 
three  whorls  and  a  D/Vol  value  less  than  0.3  suggests 
planktotrophic  development.  A  D/Vol  value  between  0.3 
and  1.0  with  less  than  three  volutions  indicates  a  species 
with  either  planktotrophic  or  non-planktotrophic  devel- 
opment, whereas  a  D/Vol  value  between  0.3  and  1.0  and 
less  than  2.25  volutions  suggests  a  species  with  a  non- 
planktotrophic  larval  type.  A  D/Vol  value  higher  than  1 .0 
would  suggest  a  species  with  direct  development  (Shuto, 
1974).  However,  Pawlik  ct  al.  (1988)  have  shown  that  the 
criteria  of  Shuto  ( 1974)  cannot  accurately  predict  the  ac- 
tual mode  of  development  in  a  majority  of  cancellariid 
gastropods. 

The  type  of  sculpture  or  ornamentation  on  the  proto- 
conch has  been  widely  used  to  infer  the  mode  of  devel- 
opment in  prosobranch  gastropods  (Thorson,  1950; 
Shuto,  1974;  Bandel,  1975a,  b,  c,  1982;  Lima  and  Lutz, 
1990).  Planktotrophy  has  been  indicated  for  those  larvae 
with  protoconchs  possessing  a  fine  reticulate  or  cancellate 
pattern,  oblique  radial  ribs  or  both,  whereas  a  smooth  or 
simply  sculptured  protoconch  suggests  that  the  larvae  are 
non-planktotrophic  (Thorson,  1950;  Shuto.  1974;  Bandel. 
1975a,  b,  c,  1982).  A  well  developed  protoconch  orna- 
mentation is  thought  to  strengthen  the  shell,  a  benefit  to 
planktotrophic  larvae  spending  lengthy  periods  in  the 
plankton  (Bandel,  1975a;  Jablonski  and  Lutz,  1980).  Two 
recent  reviews  of  poecilogony,  or  intraspecific  variation 
in  the  mode  of  larval  development,  found  no  evidence 
for  the  occurrence  of  this  phenomenon  in  prosobranch 
gastropods,  indicating  that  the  form  of  the  protoconch  is 
a  species-specific  character  (Hoagland  and  Robertson, 
1988;  Bouchet,  1989).  Nevertheless,  the  species  variability 
of  protoconch  and  teleoconch  morphologies  of  cultured 
meso-  and  neogastropods  led  Lima  and  Lutz  (1990)  to 
stress  the  need  for  caution  when  inferring  type  of  devel- 
opment from  shell  morphology  alone. 

The  most  reliable  method  for  determining  develop- 
mental mode  from  protoconch  morphologies  is  to  com- 
pare confamilial  or  congeneric  species  with  known  de- 
velopmental histories  (Scheltema,  1978;  Jablonski  and 
Lutz,  1980,  1983).  In  the  case  of  deep-sea  prosobranchs, 
the  comparison  must  be  made  with  taxonomically  related 
shallow-water  species,  the  assumption  being  that  similar 
protoconch  morphologies  result  from  similar  life  history 
patterns  in  shallow  and  deep  seas  (Colman  et  a/..  1986). 

Based  on  the  above  larval  shell  criteria,  the  majority  of 
vent  gastropods  are  believed  to  have  non-planktotrophic 
development  and  to  have  limited  larval  dispersal  capa- 
bility (Lutz  et  a/.,  1984,  1986;  Turner  et  al,  1985),  al- 
though low  temperatures  encountered  in  the  deep  sea  may 
extend  the  period  available  for  dispersal  of  swimming  but 
non-feeding  veligers  (Turner  et  al.,  1985).  This  abundance 
of  non-planktotrophy  may,  in  part,  be  due  to  the  fact  that 


36  R.  G.  GUSTAFSON  ET  AL 

Table  I 

DS\ '  "Alvin  "  Jive  number,  dale,  location,  latitude/longitude,  and  depth  oj  dive*  in  which  gastropod  egg  capsules  were  retrieved 


Dive* 


Date 


Location 


Latitude;  Longitude 


Depth  (m) 


1418 

24  July  1984 

Juan  de  Fuca  Ridge 

47°57.0'N:  129=04.0^ 

2212 

Endeavour  Segment 

1419 

25  July  1984 

Juan  de  Fuca  Ridge 

47°57.0'N;  129°04.0'W 

2208 

Endeavour  Segment 

1523 

11  March  1985 

Galapagos  Rift 

0°48.3'N;86013.5'W 

2450 

Rose  Garden  Vent 

1527 

16  March  1985 

Galapagos  Rift 

0°48.3'N;86°13.5'W 

2450 

Rose  Garden  Vent 

1528 

17  March  1985 

Galapagos  Rift 

0°48.3'N;  86°13.5'W 

2450 

Rose  Garden  Vent 

1529 

18  March  1985 

Galapagos  Rift 

0°48.3'N;86°13.5'W 

2450 

Rose  Garden  Vent 

1531 

20  March  1985 

Galapagos  Rift 

0°48.3'N;86°13.5'W 

2450 

Rose  Garden  Vent 

2031 

3  May  1988 

Galapagos  Rift 

0°48.3'N;86013.5'W 

2450 

Rose  Garden  Vent 

the  majority  of  gastropods  found  at  the  vents  are  limpet- 
like  or  coiled  archeogastropods.  When  found  in  shallow 
seas,  these  gastropods  appear  to  be  phylogenetically  con- 
strained to  non-planktotrophy  (Anderson,  1960;  Heslinga, 
1981;Strathmann,  1978a,  b;  Rex  and  Waren,  1982;Lutz 
etui..  1984;Jablonski,  1985;  Waren  and  Bouchet,  1989). 

Analysis  of  developmental  stages  contained  in  benthic 
egg  capsules  also  provides  information  about  the  life  his- 
tory of  bottom-dwelling  gastropods.  Although  many  re- 
searchers have  described  the  egg  capsules  and  encapsulated 
embryos  of  shallow-water  (see  reviews  in  Fretter  and  Gra- 
ham, 1962;  Robertson,  1976;  Webber,  1977;  Fretter,  1984; 
Pechenik,  1986;  Soliman,  1987;  M.  F.  Strathmann,  1987) 
and  deep-sea  (Thorson,  1940b;  Bouchet  and  Waren, 
1979a,  1980,  1985a,  1985b;  Colman  and  Tyler,  1988) 
marine  prosobranch  gastropods,  only  brief  mention  has 
been  made  of  the  rarely  collected  egg  capsules  from  hy- 
drothermal  vent  habitats  and  their  contents  (Turner  et 
al,,  1985;  Berg,  1985).  "Lens-shaped"  egg  capsules  mea- 
suring 10-12  mm  (Turner  et  al.,  1985)  or  17.7  ±  3.8  mm 
(Berg,  1985)  in  diameter  have  been  reported  at  the  Gal- 
apagos Rift,  while  Berg  (1985)  has  briefly  described  four 
small  prosobranch  egg  capsules  4.9  mm  in  length  by  1.7 
mm  wide  and  shaped  like  an  "inflated  triangle"  from 
Garden  of  Eden  vent  on  the  Galapagos  Rift. 

When  specimens  of  the  previously  reported  egg  capsules 
from  the  Galapagos  Rift  (Turner  et  al,  1985;  Berg,  1985) 
and  numerous  egg  capsules  from  the  Endeavour  Segment 
of  the  Juan  de  Fuca  Ridge,  came  into  our  possession,  it 
was  evident  that  a  more  detailed  study  of  these  capsules 
and  their  contents  might  yield  insights  into  the  life  his- 
tories and  the  means  of  dispersal  of  these  species.  This 
paper  is  a  description  of  the  morphology  of  three  different 
egg  capsules  from  hydrothermal  vents  and  the  embryos 


and  larvae  contained  in  those  capsules.  Inferences  are  also 
made  about  the  dispersal  capabilities  of  the  contained  lar- 
vae, and  an  attempt  is  made  to  predict  which  of  the  known 
hydrothermal  vent  gastropod  species  produced  each  cap- 
sule type. 

Materials  and  Methods 

Specimens  were  retrieved  with  the  assistance  of  DSV 
"Alvin"  during  the  dives  summarized  in  Table  I.  Egg  cap- 
sules were  collected:  ( 1 )  from  the  surfaces  of  geological 
and  biological  samples  brought  up  in  the  "Alvin"  basket 
or  in  insulated  retrieval  boxes;  (2)  from  sampling  gear  or 
markers  that  had  been  left  at  the  vents  and  later  retrieved; 
and  (3)  from  sorted  material  collected  with  a  "slurp  gun" 
attached  to  "Alvin." 

On  board  ship,  specimens  were  fixed  for  24-48  h  in 
10%  formalin  buffered  with  borax,  thoroughly  rinsed, 
transferred  to  70%  ethanol,  and  finally  to  95%  ethanol  to 
prevent  corrosion  of  larval  protoconchs.  Terminology 
used  in  egg  capsule  descriptions  follows  that  of  D'Asaro 
(1970a).  Egg  capsule  length  is  the  distance  between  the 
lateral  edges  at  the  widest  point  parallel  to  the  apical  su- 
ture; width  is  the  distance  between  the  two  sides  at  the 
widest  point  perpendicular  to  the  apical  suture;  and  height 
is  the  distance  from  the  apex  to  the  basal  membrane 
through  the  capsule's  central  axis. 

Photographs,  drawings,  and  measurements  were  made 
of  pertinent  views  of  the  capsules.  Capsules  containing 
embryos  were  then  dissected  into  two  equal  halves  and 
the  embryos  were  removed.  Both  capsules  and  free  em- 
bryos were  critical  point  dried,  placed  on  stubs,  coated 
with  approximately  400  A  of  gold-palladium,  and  ex- 
amined on  an  Hitachi  S-450  scanning  electron  micro- 
scope (SEM). 


HVDROTHERMAL  VENT  EGG  CAPSULES 


table  II 

.IvcniKc  dimensions  (mean  ±  standard  deviation)  i>l  hydrothermal  vei 
gastropod  egg  capsules  collected  on  specific  dives  o/  AST  ".  ilvin" 
Number  oj  specimens  collected  on  each  dive  are  in  parentheses 


Galapagos  Rift  Lenlicular  Egg  Capsules 

Length 
"Alvin"  Dive  #                  (mm) 

Width 
(mm) 

Height 
(mm) 

1528(2)                      14.9  ±0.8 
1529(1)                           17.4 
1531  (4)                     16.4  ±  1.3 
2031  (22)                    14.4  ±  1.7 

1  3.4  ±  0.0 
15.9 
14.0  ±0.7 
13.9  ±  1.8 

Galapagos  Rift  Inflated-Triangular 

Egg  Capsules 

Length 
"Alvin"  Dive  #                  (mm) 

Width 
(mm) 

Height 
(mm) 

1523(1)                            4.1 
1527(1)                            3.2 
1528(1)                          5.0 

1.1 
1.1 
1.6 

2.4 
3.2 
3.4 

Juan  de  Fuca  Ridge  Egg  Capsules 

Length 
"Alvin"  Dive  #                  (mm) 

Width 
(mm) 

Height 
(mm) 

1418(8)                      3.  3  ±0.6 
1419(48)                    3.7+0.5 

1.2  ±0.1 
1.4  ±0.3 

2.7  ±0.7 
3.8  ±  0.6 

Values  for  the  maximum  dimension  of  the  protoconch 
and  the  number  of  volutions  of  the  larval  shell  were  mea- 
sured directly  from  scanning  electron  micrographs  by  the 
methods  of  Shuto  (1974).  Maximum  diameter  of  the  pro- 
toconch was  denned  as  the  straight-line  distance  from  the 
protoconch-teleoconch  boundary  to  the  opposite  side  of 
the  protoconch  in  the  region  of  greatest  width.  Maximum 
diameter  of  the  Protoconch  I  was  denned  as  ( 1 )  the  greatest 


Figures  1-2.  Lenticular  egg  capsules  from  Rose  Garden  Vent  on  the 
Galapagos  Rift.  Figure  1.  Photograph  of  egg  capsules  attached  to  location 
marker  retrieved  on  "Alvin"  Dive  2031.  Scale  bar  =  50  mm.  Figure  2. 
Light  micrograph  of  apical  view  of  egg  capsule  removed  from  substrate. 
Arrow  marks  the  escape  aperture.  Scale  bar  =  5  mm.  bm,  basal  mem- 
brane. 


straight-line  distance  from  the  Protoconch  I-Protoconch 
II  boundary  in  species  with  planktotrophic  development, 
or  (2)  from  the  Protoconch  I-teleoconch  boundary  in  non- 
planktotrophic  species  lacking  a  Protoconch  II,  to  the  op- 
posite side  of  the  Protoconch  I  in  the  region  of  greatest 
width  [see  Lima  and  Lutz  (1990),  their  figure  IB,  for  a 
diagrammatic  depiction  of  these  dimensions].  Maximum 
diameter  measurements  of  the  protoconch  should  not  be 
confused  with  Robertson's  ( 197 1 )  "first  whorl  diameter," 
which  is  a  measurement  of  the  straight-line  distance  tan- 
gent to  the  straight  beginning  of  the  suture  and  extended 
in  both  directions  to  where  it  intersects  the  nearest  suture 
(Lima  and  Lutz,  1990,  their  figure  1A).  All  these  values 
are  most  accurately  determined  when  the  shell  is  viewed 
with  an  apical  orientation.  Taxonomic  terminology  and 
categories  are  in  agreement  with  those  outlined  in  Vaught 
(1989). 

Results 

The  gastropod  egg  capsules  described  in  this  report 
consist  of  three  distinct  groups:  (1)  lenticular  capsules, 
with  a  flattened  oval  or  circular  base  and  a  convex  upper 
surface,  from  the  Galapagos  Rift;  (2)  inflated  triangular- 
shaped  capsules,  from  the  Galapagos  Rift;  and  (3)  inflated 
oval  or  pouch-like  capsules  attached  to  the  substratum  by 
a  flattened  basal  membrane,  from  the  Endeavour  Segment 
of  the  Juan  de  Fuca  Ridge.  Dimensions  of  each  collected 
capsule  are  summarized  in  Table  II. 

Galapagos  Rift  lenticular  egg  capsules 

Twenty-nine  round  to  oval,  lenticular  egg  capsules  av- 
eraging 14.8  ±  1.8  mm  in  length  by  14.0  ±  1.7  mm  in 
width  were  collected  during  four  separate  dives  at  Rose 
Garden  Vent  on  the  Galapagos  Rift  (Figs.  1-3;  Tables  I, 
II).  Height  could  not  be  measured  due  to  unequal  defor- 
mation of  the  capsules  during  fixation  and  dehydration. 
Twenty-two  capsules  were  found  attached  to  a  gray  poly- 
ethylene marker  retrieved  on  "Alvin"  Dive  2031  (Fig.  2), 
while  the  remaining  specimens  were  found  attached  to 
basaltic  rocks  by  a  thin  basal  membrane  that  extends  be- 
yond the  limits  of  the  capsule  chamber  (Figs.  2,  3). 

The  outer  surface  of  the  whitish  to  transparent  capsules 
was  smooth;  there  were  no  apparent  ridges  (Figs.  2,  3).  A 
transparent  elongated  escape  aperture,  centered  about  the 
long  axis  of  each  capsule,  blended  into  an  indistinct  apical 
suture  that  effectively  separated  each  low  capsule  into  two 
equal  halves  (Figs.  2,  3).  The  capsule  wall  had  three  layers 
consisting  of  a  compact,  dense  inner  layer,  a  spongy-fi- 
brous middle  layer,  and  a  compact,  dense  outer  layer. 
(Figs.  4-5).  The  elongated  escape  aperture  was  derived 
from  a  hollow  chamber  within  the  middle  spongy-fibrous 
layer;  this  chamber  caused  the  capsule  wall  to  bulge  out- 
ward above  the  level  of  the  capsule  surface  (Fig.  5). 


38 


R.  G.  GUSTAFSON  ET  AL 


••' 


Figure  3.  ( A )  Apical  view  oflenticular  egg  capsule  from  the  Galapagos 
Rift  with  individual  embryos  visible  through  transparent  capsule  wall.  Ar- 
row marks  the  escape  aperture.  Scale  bar  =  5  mm.  bm,  basal  membrane. 
(B)  Lateral  view  of  lenticular  egg  capsule  with  peripheral  extension  of  basal 
membrane.  Arrow  marks  the  escape  aperture.  Scale  bar  =  5  mm.  (C) 
Group  of  four  lenticular  capsules  drawn  as  they  appeared  attached  to  lo- 
cation marker  in  Figure  1.  prior  to  fixation.  Scale  bar  =  5  mm. 


Early  trochophore  and  veliger  larvae  in  various  stages 
of  development  were  present  in  capsules  collected  during 
"Alvin"  Dives  1528,  1529,  and  1531  (Figs.  6-11):  one 
capsule  collected  during  Dive  2031  contained  1052  veliger 
larvae,  all  with  a  fully  formed  Protoconch  I  (Figs.  12-14). 
All  other  lenticular  capsules  collected  during  Dive  2031 
were  empty.  No  nurse  eggs  were  observed  in  lenticular 
egg  capsules. 

The  following  is  a  chronological  reconstruction  of  de- 
velopmental stages  found  in  a  number  oflenticular  cap- 
sules from  the  Galapagos  Rift.  The  earliest  stage  encoun- 
tered, a  late  prototroch,  was  approximately  175  ^m  in 
length  by  100  /um  in  width  (as  measured  from  electron 
micrographs),  with  a  prominent  apical  plate,  short  pre- 
trochal  region,  prototroch.  long  post-trochal  region, 
mouth,  and  very  early  larval  shell  (Figs.  6-10).  The  apical 
plate  lacked  an  apical  ciliary  tuft  while  the  prototrochal 
cilia  appeared  to  be  of  the  compound  type  and  15-19  //m 
in  length  (Fig.  7).  The  posterior-dorsal  shell  field  (see  Eys- 
terand  Morse,  1984,  for  terminology)  had  already  invag- 
inated  in  the  earliest  specimens  obtained,  and  some  shell 
secretion  had  commenced  (Figs.  8-10). 

The  next  observed  stage  of  development  was  a  veliger 
larva,  which  had  a  bi-lobed  velum,  a  mouth  leading  into 
the  stomadeum,  a  foot  primordium — a  protruding  knob 
located  immediately  posterior  to  the  mouth — an  oper- 
culum.  and  a  more  developed  larval  shell  (Fig.  1 1).  This 
was  a  very  early  veliger  because  the  body  was  still  much 
too  large  to  be  withdrawn  into  the  shell. 

One  capsule  collected  during  Dive  2031  (Figs.  12-14) 
contained  late  Protoconch  I  larvae  that  were  almost  ready 
to  hatch.  The  embryonic  shells  of  these  specimens  had  a 
maximum  diameter  of  234  ^m  (as  measured  from  electron 
micrographs).  The  larval  shells  of  these  larvae  had  a  fine 
reticulate  sculpture  formed  of  spiral  raised  ridges  running 
in  the  direction  of  growth,  and  crossed  by  regularly  spaced 
perpendicular  riblets  (Figs.  12-14).  An  uncalcified  oper- 
culum  was  present  at  this  stage  (Fig.  13). 

Galapagos  Rift  inflated  triangular  egg  capsules 

Three  specimens  of  an  egg  capsule  4. 1  ±  0.9  mm  in 
length  by  1.3  ±  0.3  mm  in  width  by  3.0  ±  0.5  mm  in 
height  and  shaped  like  an  inflated  triangle  were  found 
attached  to  basaltic  substrates  during  a  series  of  "Alvin" 
dives  at  the  Galapagos  Rift  in  1985  (Figs.  15,  16;  Table 
II).  Capsules  were  attached  by  a  basal  membrane  that 
barely  extends  beyond  the  limits  of  the  capsule  chamber 
(Fig.  16).  A  lateral  ridge  extended  up  from  either  end  of 
the  long  axis  of  these  capsules  to  meet  at  the  capsule's 
slightly  off-center  apex  (Figs.  15,  16).  Except  for  the 
prominent  lateral  ridge,  the  surfaces  of  these  capsules  were 
smooth.  Capsules  fixed  in  10%  buttered  formalin  and 
subsequently  stored  in  ethanol,  ranged  in  color  from  white 


HYDROTHERMAL  VENT  EGG  CAPSULES 


Figures  4-5.  Scanning  electron  micrographs  of  lenticular  egg  capsule  wall  from  Galapagos  Rit't.  Figure 
4.  Cross-section  of  capsule  wall.  Scale  bar  =  10  nm.  ow.  outer  capsule  wall;  iw.  inner  capsule  wall;  si.  spongy 
layer.  Figure  5.  Cross-section  through  the  escape  aperture  chamber.  The  outer  surface  of  the  capsule  is 
towards  the  top.  Scale  bar  =  100  /im. 


to  yellowish-white  to  almost  orange.  The  capsule  wall  was 
composed  of  what  appeared  to  be  one  spongy-fibrous  layer 
(Fig.  17). 

Each  capsule  contained  several  hundred  early  veliger 
larvae  approximately  165  /urn  in  length  by  98  /urn  in  width, 
as  measured  from  electron  micrographs.  Larvae  in  all 
three  capsules  were  at  the  same  relative  stage  of  devel- 
opment and  were  characterized  by  a  bi-lobed  velum,  an 
apical  sensory  region  with  cephalic  cilia,  a  mouth,  a  foot 
primordium  with  attached  operculum,  and  an  early  Pro- 
toconch  I  (Figs.  18,  19).  Velar  compound  cilia  were  ap- 
proximately 30  urn  long.  The  early  protoconch  was  over- 
laid by  a  membrane  that  obscured  a  sculpture  of  radially 
arranged  rows  of  short  tubercules  intersected  by  weak 
concentric  raised  ridges  or  lines  (Figs.  19-21).  Distal  to 
this  membrane,  the  sculpture  consisted  of  parallel  raised 
ridges  running  in  the  direction  of  growth,  crossed  by  radial 
riblets,  and  forming  a  cancellate  or  net-like  pattern  (Fig. 
19).  Nurse  eggs  were  not  present  in  the  three  inflated  tri- 
angular egg  capsules. 

Juan  de  Fuca  Ridge  egg  capsules 

Fifty-six  orange  egg  capsules,  each  shaped  like  an  in- 
flated oval  or  pouch  and  measuring  3.6  ±  0.5  mm  in 
length,  1.3  ±  0.3  mm  in  width,  and  3.6  ±  0.7  mm  in 
height  were  collected  during  "Alvin"  Dives  14 18  and  1419 
on  the  Endeavour  Segment  of  the  Juan  de  Fuca  Ridge  in 
1984  (Tables  I,  II).  Each  capsule  was  attached  to  the  sub- 
strate by  a  flattened  basal  membrane  (Figs.  22,  23).  A 
lateral  ridge  rose  abruptly  from  the  thin  basal  membrane 
at  either  end  of  the  capsule.  About  2  mm  above  the  sub- 
stratum, the  ridges  at  either  end  of  the  capsule  split  into 


two  wing-like  extensions  forming  a  saddle-shaped  struc- 
ture around  the  central  oval  escape  aperture  (Fig.  23B). 
In  most  cases  an  amorphous,  poorly  fixed,  orange  em- 
bryonic mass,  containing  an  indeterminate  number  of 
embryos,  occupied  the  capsule  chamber  (Figs.  22,  23C). 
In  other  cases,  from  one  to  six,  but  most  often  five,  larvae 
were  observed  through  the  capsule  walls.  The  Juan  de 
Fuca  Ridge  egg  capsule  wall  consisted  of  two  compact 
dense  layers:  an  outer  and  an  inner  layer  separated  by  a 
sharp  boundary  (Fig.  24). 

Examination  of  the  amorphous  yolk  mass  present  in 
most  capsules  revealed  that  some  larval  shell  had  been 
secreted,  but  structural  details  were  indeterminable.  Nurse 
eggs  may  have  been  present,  but  fixation  was  too  poor 
for  this  to  be  determined.  However,  more  advanced  larvae 
were  present  in  a  few  capsules,  which  revealed  a  paucispi- 
ral  protoconch  that  was  large  and  bulbous  and  lacked 
ornamentation  other  than  that  due  to  weak  growth  lines 
(Figs.  25,  26). 

Discussion 

Although  some  archeogastropods  embed  their  eggs  in 
a  benthic  gelatinous  mass  or  ribbon,  the  majority  of  shal- 
low-water archeogastropods  do  not  produce  benthic  egg 
capsules  (Fretter  and  Graham,  1962;Hyman,  1967;  Rob- 
ertson, 1976;  Webber.  1977;  Bandel,  1982;  Fretter,  1984; 
Soliman,  1987;  M.  F.  Strathmann,  1987).  Therefore,  egg 
capsules  described  in  this  paper  from  hydrothermal  vents 
are  most  likely  the  spawn  of  prosobranchs  of  the  higher 
orders  Mesogastropoda  or  Neogastropoda.  Various  au- 
thors (Anderson,  1960;  Amio,  1963;  Bandel,  1976a,  b; 
Soliman,  1987)  have  stressed  that  the  general  form  of  the 


Figures  6-10.  Scanning  electron  micrographs  of  early  trochophore  larvae  removed  from  Galapagos  Rifl 
lenticular  egg  capsules.  Figure  6.  Ventral  view  showing  apical  plate  (ap).  prototroch  (pt),  and  mouth  (m). 
Scale  bar  =  25  nm.  Figure  7.  Apical  view  showing  apical  plate  (ap)  and  prototroch  (pt).  Scale  bar  =  20  /im. 
Figure  8.  Early  protoconch  at  extreme  posterior  end.  Scale  bar  =  10  /im.  Figure  9.  Right  lateral  aspect 
showing  apical  plate  (ap).  prototroch  (pt),  and  protoconch  (pc).  Scale  bar  =  25  urn.  Figure  10.  Left  lateral 
aspect  of  different  specimen  to  that  shown  in  Figure  9.  Scale  as  in  Figure  9.  ap.  apical  plate;  pt,  prototroch: 

pc,  protoconch. 

40 


Figures  1 1-14.  Scanning  electron  micrographs  of  early  and  late  veliger  larvae  extracted  from  lenticular 
egg  capsules  from  the  Galapagos  Rift.  Figure  II.  Early  veliger  larva  showing  apical  plate  (ap),  velum  (v), 
mouth  (m).  foot  primordium  (f),  operculum  (o).  and  protoconch  (pc).  Scale  bar  =  25  p.m.  Figure  12.  Apical 
view  of  Protoconch  I  in  larva  near  hatching.  Scale  bar  =  50  urn.  Figure  13.  Apertural  view  of  Protoconch 
1  in  larva  near  hatching.  Scale  bar  =  25  jim.  o.  operculum.  Figure  14.  Ventral  view  of  Protoconch  I  in  larva 
near  hatching.  Scale  bar  =  50  MHI. 

41 


42 


R.  G.  GUSTAFSON  KT  A  I. 


Figure  15.  Light  micrograph  of  convex  side  of  Galapagos  Rift  inflated 
triangular  egg  capsule  with  embryos  visible  through  the  transparent  cap- 
sule wall.  Arrows  mark  the  lateral  ridges.  Scale  bar  =  1  mm.  bm.  basal 
membrane. 


oothecae  in  different  gastropod  taxa  is  characteristic  of 
the  species,  and  in  some  cases,  of  higher  orders  of  clas- 
sification, and  may  be  valuable  in  taxonomy.  It  should 
be  noted,  however,  that  similar  capsules  may  be  produced 
by  taxonomically  diverse  species,  while  in  other  cases  in- 
terspecific variation  in  capsule  morphology  is  insufficient 
to  differentiate  closely  related  species  (Kohn.  1961 ). 


Figure  16.  (A)  View  of  the  convex  side  of  Galapagos  Rift  inflated 
triangular  egg  capsule  with  individual  embryos  visible  through  transparent 
membrane.  Arrows  mark  the  lateral  ridges.  Scale  bar  =  1  mm.  (B)  Apical 
view  of  Galapagos  Rift  inflated  triangular  egg  capsule.  Arrows  mark  the 
lateral  ridges.  Scale  bar  =  1  mm.  bm.  basal  membrane. 


Galapagos  Rift  lenticular  egg  capsules 

Flattened  lenticular  egg  capsules  with  a  centrally  located 
escape  aperture  are  known  from  the  neogastropod  families 
Muricidae,  Fasciolariidae,  and  Turridae.  Dimensions  and 
other  statistics  pertaining  to  selected  lenticular  egg  capsules 
from  these  families  are  presented  in  Table  III.  The  only 
member  of  these  families  known  to  occur  at  the  Galapagos 
Rift  hydrothermal  vents  is  a  large  turrid,  provisionally 
described  as  Phymorhynchus  sp.  (Waren  and  Bouchet, 
1 989).  A  similar  species  occurs  at  1 3°N  and  2 1  °N  on  the 
East  Pacific  Rise  (Turner  el  a!.,  1985;  Waren  and  Bouchet, 
1989).  Both  the  six  egg  capsules  described  by  Turner  el 
al.  (1985)  and  the  five  "lens-shaped"  egg  cases  described 
by  Berg  (1985)  as  characteristic  of  turrids,  as  well  as,  the 
lenticular  egg  capsules  described  in  this  paper,  may  all 
belong  to  Phymorhynchus  sp.  from  the  Galapagos  Rift. 
Differences  in  reported  average  size  between  these  three 
groups  of  capsules  is  not  unexpected,  because  capsule  size 
in  neogastropods  is  proportional  to  adult  size.  Capsule 
size  is  also  correlated  with  female  foot  width;  the  capsule 
is  formed  and  manipulated  by  the  foot  during  deposition 
(Robertson,  1976;  Shimek,  1986). 

Berg ( 1985)  estimated  that  "lens-shaped"  oothecae  from 
the  Garden  of  Eden  and  Mussel  Bed  hydrothermal  vent 
sites  along  the  Galapagos  Rift  contained  from  500-1000 


eggs  with  a  mean  size  of  192.1  ±  13.5  nm  by  136.2  ±10.1 
nm.  This  agrees  well  with  our  count  of  1052  larvae  in  one 
capsule  from  Dive  203 1  and  with  the  size  of  larvae  both 
from  this  capsule  (234  ^m  maximum  diameter)  and  from 


Figure  17.  Scanning  electron  micrograph  of  cross-section  of  single- 
layered  spongy  capsule  wall  of  inflated  triangular  egg  capsule  from  Gal- 
apagos Rift.  The  outer  surface  is  towards  the  top.  Scale  bar  =  5  ^m. 


HYDROTHERMAL  VENT  EGG  CAPSULES 


43 


Figures  18-21.  Scanning  electron  micrographs  of  veliger  larvae  extracted  from  inflated  triangular  egg 
capsules  from  Galapagos  Rift.  Figure  18.  Lateral  ventral  view  showing  velum  (v).  apical  plate  (ap).  mouth 
(m).  foot  primordium  (f).  and  early  protoconch  (pc).  Scale  bar  =  25  urn.  Figure  19.  Lateral  view  showing 
cancellate  or  net-like  early  protoconch  (pc)  sculpture  and  obscuring  membrane  (me).  Scale  bar  =  25  urn. 
o.  operculum:  v.  velum.  Figure  20.  Apical  view  of  early  protoconch.  Scale  bar  =  20  urn.  Figure  21.  Dorsal 
view  of  early  protoconch.  Scale  bar  =  20  ^m. 


capsules  with  larvae  in  earlier  stages  of  development  ( 1 75 
/um  in  length  by  100  ^m  in  width).  The  absence  of  nurse 
eggs  further  suggests  these  capsules  were  laid  by  a  turrid, 
because  nurse  eggs  are  unknown  in  the  Turridae  (Table  III). 


Although  the  basal  diameter  of  lenticular  egg  capsules 
described  herein  (14.8  X  14  mm)  is  larger  than  the  2-6 
mm  of  normal  turrid  egg  capsules,  it  is  not  unprecedented. 
Egg  capsules  of  the  turrid  Mangelia  plicosa  are  30-33 


44 


R.  G.  GUSTAFSON  ET  AL. 


Figure  22.  View  of  the  convex  side  of  Juan  de  Fuca  Ridge  pouch- 
like  egg  capsule  containing  amorphous  embryonic  mass.  Scale  bar  =  1 
mm.  bm,  basal  membrane. 


mm  in  diameter,  while  those  ofPolystira  harretli  measure 
up  to  10.7  mm  in  basal  diameter  (Table  III).  Furthermore, 
the  large  size  of  the  Galapagos  Rift  turrid  (up  to  74  mm 
in  height,  pers.  obs.,  RGG)  is  consistent  with  the  large 
size  of  the  egg  capsules.  Although  turrid  egg  capsule  wall 
structure  has  not  been  previously  studied,  the  three-lay- 
ered capsule  wall  (Fig.  4)  is  similar  to  that  described  for 
the  closely  related  Conidae  (D'Asaro,  1988).  This  is  con- 
sistent with  the  designation  of  these  capsules  as  belonging 
to  a  turrid,  because  lenticular  capsules  of  the  Muricoidea 
have  four  layers  (D'Asaro,  1988).  Some  degree  of  repro- 
ductive synchrony  may  occur  in  the  population  of  the 
Galapagos  Rift  turrid  provisionally  described  as  Pliymo- 
rhvnchus  sp.,  because  eggs,  embryos,  and  larvae  contained 
in  different  lenticular  capsules  collected  during  March 
1985  and  May  1988  in  the  present  study  (Tables  I,  II), 
and  by  Berg  (1985),  were  at  the  same  relative  stage  of 
development  on  each  collection  date  but  were  at  different 
stages  of  development  between  collection  dates. 

Galapagos  Rift  inflated  triangular  capsules 

Berg's  (1985)  account  of  four  egg  capsules,  each  "shaped 
like  a  small  inflated  triangle,"  retrieved  from  a  larval  trap 
in  1979  at  the  Garden  of  Eden  vent  on  the  Galapagos 
Rift,  agrees  in  every  respect  with  the  description  found 
here  of  the  inflated  triangular  capsules  from  Rose  Garden 
vent.  Although  Berg  (1985)  does  not  give  a  measure  of 
the  egg  capsule's  height,  the  average  length  of  4.9  mm 
and  width  of  1.7  mm  of  his  capsules  is  similar  to  the 
average  length  of  4.1  mm  and  width  of  1.3  mm  for  the 
three  inflated  triangular  capsules  described  in  this  report. 


Inflated  triangular  capsules  from  the  Galapagos  Rift 
vent  fields  are  similar  in  morphology,  but  not  in  size,  to 
those  from  5480  m  in  the  Kermadec  Trench  illustrated 
by  Bouchet  and  Waren  ( 1 985a)  and  attributed  to  the  buc- 
cinid  Calliloconcha  knudseni  Bouchet  and  Waren.  These 
1 5  mm  long  by  1 2  mm  high  C.  knudseni  capsules  were 
empty  and  had  been  drilled  by  a  predator.  Certain  small 
capsules  similar  to  the  inflated  triangular  capsules  found 
at  the  Galapagos  Rift,  but  without  a  prominent  lateral 
and  apical  ridge,  are  produced  by  members  of  the  neo- 
gastropod  family  Columbellidae  (Petit  and  Risbec,  1929; 
Thorson,  1 940a;  Bacci,  1947;  Amio.  1955;  Bandel,  1974a). 
Capsules  of  this  group  contain  no  more  than  60  eggs, 
which  are  usually  reduced  in  number  through  oophagy 
(Bandel.  1974a).  Small  capsules  attributed  to  the  buccinid 
Tacita  danielsseni  (Friele)  and  to  the  turrid  Oenopota 
ovalis  (Friele)  from  abyssal  parts  of  the  Norwegian  Sea 
resemble  the  inflated  triangular  capsules  in  size  but  appear 
to  lack  the  lateral  ridge  and  strong  off-center  apex  (Bouchet 
and  Waren.  1 979a).  The  egg  capsule  attributed  to  O.  ovalis 
in  Bouchet  and  Waren  (1979a,  Fig.  15)  is  unlike  any 
known  for  the  genus  Oenopota  (Thorson,  1935;  Shimek. 
1983b,  1986)  or  for  any  other  turrid.  Capsules  belonging 
to  T.  danielsseni  contained  thousands  of  small  eggs,  al- 
though only  one  large  embryo  (4  mm  in  maximum  di- 
mension) developed  in  each  capsule.  A  large  protoconch 
(840-880  /urn)  is  also  present  in  O  ovalis,  which  is  indic- 
ative of  direct  development  (Bouchet  and  Waren,  1979a: 
Rex  and  Waren,  1982).  By  comparison  to  the  above  spe- 
cies, several  hundred  veligers  were  present  in  each  of  the 
triangular  capsules  from  the  Galapagos  Rift  examined  in 
this  study.  Neither  a  buccinid  nor  a  columbellid  has  as 
yet  been  reported  from  the  Galapagos  Rift  hydrothermal 
vents. 

Gastropods  that  are  large  enough  to  have  laid  these 
capsules  at  the  Galapagos  Rift  vent  sites  include  Phy- 
nuirhvnchus  sp.,  Provanna  ios  Waren  and  Bouchet  and 
P.  muricata  Waren  and  Bouchet  (Waren  and  Bouchet, 
1986,  1989).  The  turrid  Phymorhynchus  is  an  unlikely 
candidate  because  the  capsule  type  for  this  species  has 
been  provisionally  assigned  in  the  present  study.  Although 
the  inflated  triangular  capsules  from  the  Galapagos  Rift 
are  unlike  the  typical  lenticular  capsules  of  turrids,  a  sim- 
ilar capsule  from  the  deep-sea  has  been  attributed  to  the 
turrid  Oenopota  ovalis  (Bouchet  and  Waren,  1979a;  see 
discussion  above).  Either  species  of  Provanna  is  also  an 
unlikely  choice,  because  the  recent  placement  of  Provanna 
within  the  Littorinoidea  (Waren  and  Bouchet,  1989)  sug- 
gests that  the  production  of  such  an  elaborate  egg  capsule 
is  unlikely,  because  all  known  littorinoids  spawn  either  a 
benthic,  amorphous  gelatinous  mass,  a  single  pelagic  cap- 
sule containing  a  single  egg,  or  release  veligers  or  fully 
formed  juveniles  from  an  internal  brood  pouch  (Thorson, 
1946;  Anderson.  1960,  1962;  Amio,  1963;  Pilkington, 


HYDROTHERMAL  VENT  EGG  CAPSULES 


45 


Figure  23.  (A)  View  of  the  convex  side  of  Juan  de  Fuca  Ridge  egg 
capsule.  Scale  bar  =  1  mm.  bm.  basal  membrane.  (B)  Lateral  view  of 
Juan  de  Fuca  capsule  showing  lateral  ridge  running  into  two  apical  wing- 


1974;  Robertson.  1976;  Bandel,  1974b,  1975b,  1982;  So- 
liman,  1987;  M.  F.  Strathmann,  1987).  Based  on  egg  cap- 
sule morphology,  we  cannot  assign  the  inflated  triangular 
egg  capsules  from  the  Galapagos  Rift  to  a  particular  spe- 
cies. A  more  definitive  statement  on  the  taxonomic  affil- 
iation of  the  inflated  triangular  capsule  must  await  ad- 
ditional collections  of  organisms  from  the  Galapagos  Rift 
vent  fields  and  further  taxonomic  examination  of  existing 
material. 

The  wall  structure  of  the  inflated  triangular  egg  capsules 
from  the  Galapagos  Rift  is  similar  to  that  seen  in  several 
species  within  the  Muricoidea  (Roller  and  Stickle,  1988; 
D'Asaro,  1988).  although  too  little  is  known  of  egg  capsule 
wall  structures  at  this  time  to  make  a  definitive  statement 
as  to  this  capsule's  affinity.  Because  all  three  inflated  tri- 
angular capsules  collected  from  Galapagos  Rift  on  three 
separate  dives  in  1985  contained  early  veligers  at  the  same 
stage  of  development,  it  is  possible  that  some  degree  of 
reproductive  synchrony  occurs  in  this  population. 

Juan  dc  Fuca  Ridge  egg  capsules 

Egg  capsules  strikingly  similar  in  size  and  shape  to  the 
Juan  de  Fuca  Ridge  capsules  described  in  the  present  study 
are  produced  by  the  cancellariid  neogastropod,  Admele 
viridula  (Fabricius)  [Thorson,  1935:  fig.  71  (mistakenly 
attributed  to  I  'eliitina  undala  Brown,  see  Thorson,  1944: 
108);  Bouchet  and  Waren,  1985b:  fig.  687].  The  capsules 
described  herein  and  those  of  A.  virudula  both  possess 
parallel  wing-like  extensions,  a  flattened  base,  and  an  api- 
cal escape  aperture.  Species  ofAdmete  have  been  described 
from  6700  m  deep  in  the  Kermadec  Trench,  whereas  A. 
viridula,  which  is  circumpolar,  has  a  depth  range  of  4- 
2,295  m,  according  to  Clarke  (1962).  The  small  number 
(1-6)  of  large  larvae  present  in  Juan  de  Fuca  capsules  is 
consistent  with  the  6-7  larvae  per  capsule  found  in  Adnielc 
viridula  by  Thorson  (1935)  and  the  6  larvae  per  capsule 
found  in  Admele  sp.  by  MacGinitie  (1955).  However,  a 
species  of  Admele  has  not  been  collected  from  the  Juan 
de  Fuca  Ridge. 

Other  pouch-like  egg  capsules,  with  or  without  a  flat- 
tened base,  but  without  wing-like  extensions,  are  found 
in  the  neogastropod  families  Muricidae  and  Buccinidae 
(Thorson,  1935,  1940b;  Anderson.  1960;  Golikov,  1961: 
Cowan,  1964;  Radwin  and  Chamberlin,  1973;  Macintosh, 
1979,  1986;  D'Asaro,  1986).  However,  buccinids  typically 
form  their  capsules  into  clusters  (Thorson,  1935;  Cowan, 
1964),  in  contrast  to  the  pouch-like  capsules  from  Juan 


like  extensions  (arrows)  forming  a  saddle-like  structure.  Scale  bar  =  1 
mm.  (C)  View  of  the  convex  side  of  Juan  de  Fuca  egg  capsule  with 
amorphous  embryonic  mass  visible  through  transparent  capsule  wall. 
Scale  bar  =  1  mm.  bm,  basal  membrane. 


46 


R.  G.  GUSTAFSON  ET  AL 


Figure  24.  Scanning  electron  micrograph  of  cross-section  of  double- 
layered  capsule  wall  of  Juan  de  Fuca  Ridge  egg  capsule.  Scale  bar  =  5 
^m.  ow,  outer  wall;  iw,  inner  wall. 


de  Fuca,  which  are  individually  attached  to  the  substrate. 
Capsules  secreted  by  members  of  the  neogastropod  family 
Columbellidae  frequently  possess  apical  collars  that  sur- 


round a  central  escape  aperture  somewhat  similar  to  the 
wing-like  extensions  seen  in  capsules  from  the  Juan  de 
Fuca  Ridge  described  in  the  present  study  (Thorson, 
1940a;  Perry  and  Schwengel.  1955;  Amio,  1955,  1963; 
Marcus  and  Marcus,  1962;  Scheltema,  1969;  D'Asaro, 
1970b;  Bandel,  1974a). 

Only  two  gastropods,  Buccinum  viridum  Dall  and  Pro- 
vanna  variabilis  Waren  and  Bouchet,  which  are  large 
enough  to  have  secreted  these  capsules,  have  been  re- 
corded from  the  Juan  de  Fuca  Ridge  system  (Waren  and 
Bouchet,  1986;  Tunnicliffe  el  a/.,  1985;  Tunnicliffe  and 
Fontaine,  1987;  Tunnicliffe,  1988).  Dall  (1890)  described 
B  viridum  as  having  a  maximum  shell  height  of  46  mm, 
while  P.  variabilis  reaches  a  maximum  height  of  8.7  mm 
(Waren  and  Bouchet.  1986).  Waren  and  Bouchet  (1989) 
have  recently  placed  P.  variabilis  within  the  Superfamily 
Littorinoidea.  No  members  of  the  Littorinoidea  have  as 
yet  been  shown  to  produce  complex  egg  capsules  (see  dis- 
cussion above  of  inflated  triangular  capsules),  which  sug- 
gests that  P.  variabilis  is  not  the  source  of  the  egg  capsule 
from  the  Juan  de  Fuca  Ridge.  The  Buccinidae  produce 
egg  capsules  either  singly  or  in  clusters,  each  containing 
many  nurse  eggs;  only  a  few  of  these  survive  (Thorson, 
1935,  1946;Lebour,  1937;  Anderson.  1960).  Encapsulated 


Figures  25-26.  Scanning  electron  micrographs  ol  larvae  extracted  from  Juan  de  Fuca  Ridge  egg  capsules. 
Figure  25.  Lateral  view  of  protoconch  showing  height  of  the  first  whorl  and  weak  concentric  growth  lines. 
Scale  bar  =  200  ^m.  Figure  26.  Apical  view  showing  unsculptured  appearance  of  protoconch.  Scale  bar 
=  200  urn. 


HYDROTHERMAL  VENT  EGG  CAPSULES 


47 


Table  III 

Taxonomic  affiliation,  dimension.'!,  number  of  eggs,  egg  si:e.  and  number  of  veligers  present  at  hatchint> 
for  lenticular  egg  cases  as  reported  in  selected  references 


Size  (mm) 

Number  of 
veligers  at 
hatching 

Post-hatching 
development 
type 

Reference 

Species 

Basal 
diameter 

Number 
Height           of  eggs 

Egg  size 

(Mil) 

Order  Neogastropoda 

Muricoidea 

Family  Muricidae 

Bedeva  hanleyi  (Angas) 

3.0 

50-70 

250 

15 

N 

Anderson,  1965 

Bedevina  (Lataxiena)  biri/effi 

(Lischke) 

3.0 

0.85              60-90 

190 

— 

P 

Amio,  1963 

Ergalalax  constractus  (Reeve) 

2.5-3.0 

130 

— 

— 

9 

Habe.  1960 

Ergalatax  calcareus  (Dunker) 

3.5-4.5 

130 

— 

— 

9 

Habe,  1960 

Trophon  clalhrants  (L.) 

6-7 

—                 — 

— 

9-18 

N 

Thorson.  1940b 

Trophon  muricaliis  (Montagu) 

2.5 

—                 — 

— 

2-9 

N 

Lebour.  1936 

Trophon  tnincalus  (Strom.) 

1.8-3.1 

—                 — 

— 

6-11 

N 

Thorson,  1946 

Zeairophon  (\ymene)  amhiguiis 

(Philippi) 

6-10 

1.0-1.5 

— 

600 

N-P 

Pilkington,  1974 

Family  Fasciolariidae 

Glaphyrina  vulpia  >/<  >r 

(Sowerby) 

12 

—                 — 

— 

10 

N 

Pilkington,  1974 

Conoidea 

Family  Turridae 

Clavus  japonicus  (Lischke) 

2.5 

1.8                  2-4 

650 

2-4 

N 

Amio,  1963 

Crassispira  sp. 

1.5 

—                2-5 

— 

2-5 

N 

Bandel.  1976b 

Drillia  crenu/aris  (Lamarck) 

6-7 

150-170 

230-300 

150-170 

P 

Thorson,  1940a 

Drillia  so/ida  (C.  B.  Adams) 

4 

1                     2-7 

— 

2-7 

9 

Bandel,  1976b 

Kurliiella  plumbea  (Hinds) 

2.3  ±  0.3 

180  ±43 

137  ±    8 

180  +  43 

9 

Shimek.  1983c 

Mangelia  nebula  (Montagu) 

1.6 





60 

P 

Lebour,  1914,  1916.  1917 

Mungelia  plicosa  (C.  B.  Adams) 

30.1-33.0 

0.32                 60 

160 

9 

Perry  and  Schwengel,  1955 

Oenopoia  simplex  (Midd.) 

2-3 

—                 — 

— 

5-6 

N 

Thorson.  1935 

Oenopota  exarata  (Moller) 

3.0-4.5 

—                 — 

— 

5-21 

N 

Thorson,  1935 

Oenopota  bicarinata  (Couth.) 

2.25-3.25 

—                 — 

— 

3-11 

N 

Thorson.  1935 

Oenopota  pyramidialis  (Strom.) 

3.5-6 

—                 — 

— 

4-20 

N 

Thorson,  1935 

Oenopota  nohilis  (Moller) 

4.5-4.75 

—                 — 

— 

3-7 

N 

Thorson.  1935 

Oenopota  trevelyana  (Tunon) 

3.1-3.3 

—                 — 

— 

25-3  1 

N-P 

Thorson,  1946 

Oenopota  turricola  (Montagu) 

2.5 

100-150 

— 

100-150 

9 

Vestergaard.  1935 

Oenopoia  elegans  (Moller) 

3.02 

250 

150 

250 

P 

Shimek.  I983b 

Oenopoia  excurvata  (Carpenter) 

2.08 

1  .08                 30 

212 

— 

9 

Shimek,  1983b 

Oenopota  jtdiciila  (Gould) 

2.24 

0.98                 20 

371 

— 

9 

Shimek,  I983b 

Oenopoia  levidensis  (Carpenter) 

5.30 

1.38            175  ±85 

286 

175  ±85 

P 

Shimek,  1983b 

Ophiodermella  inermis  (Hinds) 

4.68  ±  0.99 

1.44  ±0.29     208  ±61 

222  ±  15 

208  ±  61 

P 

Shimek,  1983a 

Philberlia  gracilis  (Montagu) 

3.4 





40-80 

P 

Lebour   1914   1916    1917 

Philbertia  lineari.s  (Montagu) 

1.5-2.0 

60-80 

140-150 

P 

Lebour.  1934.  1936,  1937 

— 

51-114 

150 

— 

P 

Thorson,  1946 

Philberlia  purpurea  (Montagu) 

5.3 

0.6             350-400 

100 

— 

P 

Franc.  1950 

Polystira  barrelti  (Guppy) 

5.0-10.7 

32-126 

438 

32-126 

N 

Penchaszadeh,  1982 

Raphitoma  (Teretia)  amoena 

(Sars) 

1.25-1.5 

—                 — 

— 

2 

N 

Thorson.  1935 

Unknown  turrid 

2-3.5 

—                 — 

— 

— 

9 

Arnaud  and  Zibrowius,  1973 

Unknown  turrid 

2.6 

—                 — 

— 

— 

9 

Bouchet  and  Waren.  1980 

Unknown  turrid 

4.0-4.2 

—                 — 

— 

— 

9 

Bouchet  and  Waren.  1980 

direct  development  is  universal  within  this  group  (Lebour. 
1937;  Robertson,  1976;  Colman  et  at..  1986;  Waren  and 
Bouchet,  1989),  although  planktotrophic  buccinids  are 
known  from  the  Early  Tertiary  (Hansen,  1982).  Capsule 


wall  morphology  ofthe  Juan  de  Fuca  egg  capsule  described 
in  the  present  study  is  similar  to  that  seen  in  certain  Buc- 
cinidae  and  M uricidae  ( D' Asaro,  1988).  although  a  defin- 
itive statement  is  not  possible  until  the  micromorphology 


48 


R.  G.  GUSTAFSON  ET  AL 


of  a  wider  taxonomic  grouping  of  capsules  is  known.  On 
the  basis  of  egg  capsule  morphology  we  are  unable  to  as- 
sign the  pouch-like  egg  capsules  from  the  Juan  de  Fuca 
Ridge  to  any  species  that  has  as  yet  been  collected  from 
this  site.  However,  the  similarity  of  the  Juan  de  Fuca  Ridge 
capsules  to  those  produced  by  species  of  Admclc  (Thorson, 
1935;  Bouchet  and  Waren,  1985b)  is  so  striking  that  it 
leads  us  to  predict  that  a  member  of  this  genus  may  soon 
be  found  associated  with  this  hydrothermal  vent. 

Egg  size,  fecundity,  and  protoconch  morphology 

The  size  of  the  ovum  in  those  prosobranch  gastropods 
that  do  not  provide  nurse  eggs  or  albumen  in  the  egg 
capsule  regulates  the  amount  of  nutrition  supplied  to  the 
larva  and  has  a  close  relationship  with  the  type  of  larval 
development  [Thorson  1946,  1950:Shuto,  1974;  Bandel, 
1975a;  Lima  and  Lutz.  1990;  but  see  discussion  on  this 
topic  in  Vance  (1973,  1974),  Underwood  (1974),  Steele 
(1977),  Strathmann  (1977),  Perron  (1981),  Todd  and 
Doyle  (1981),  and  Hines,  (1986)].  Planktotrophic  pro- 
sobranch gastropod  larvae  typically  have  a  small  pointed 
apex,  often  with  delicate  sculpture,  reflecting  an  originally 
small  ovum.  However,  non-planktotrophic  larvae  typi- 
cally have  a  large  rounded  apex  reflecting  a  large  ovum 
with  plenty  of  yolk  available  for  the  larva  to  grow  to  a 
large  size  (Ockelmann,  1965;  Shuto,  1974).  This  relation- 
ship may  become  obscured  in  those  non-planktotrophs 
that  feed  on  nurse  eggs  or  other  forms  of  extraembryonic 
nutrition  and  emerge  as  large  juveniles,  because  they  often 
have  egg  diameters  no  larger  than  those  of  free-swimming, 
non-planktotrophs  (Bandel.  1975a,  c;  Jablonski,  1986). 
Similarly,  Hadfield  and  Strathmann  (1990)  have  shown 
that  egg  size  is  not  a  reliable  indicator  for  differentiating 
between  a  pelagic  and  a  benthic  mode  of  development  in 
non-planktotrophic  trochoidean  archeogastropods.  In 
addition,  although  archeogastropods  are  purported  to  be 
exclusively  non-planktotrophic  (Anderson,  1960;  Hes- 
linga,  1981;  Strathmann,  1978a,  b;  Rex  and  Waren,  1982; 
Lutz  et  al.,  1984;  Jablonski,  1985;  Waren  and  Bouchet, 
1989),  many  species  develop  from  relatively  small  eggs 
between  1 10  and  230  ^m  in  diameter  (Amio,  1963),  and 
as  small  as  80  p.m  in  some  cases  (Bandel,  1982).  Unfor- 
tunately, egg  size  criteria  could  not  be  applied  in  the  pres- 
ent study,  because  ova  were  not  encountered. 

In  the  absence  of  nurse  eggs,  the  number  of  eggs  pro- 
duced by  a  gastropod  species  is  also  indicative  of  its  mode 
of  larval  development  (Thorson,  1950;  Crisp,  1978;  Shuto, 
1974;  Bandel,  1975a).  A  large  number  of  eggs  suggests 
planktotrophic  development,  because  many  free-swim- 
ming embryos  and  larvae  are  assumed  to  be  lost  to  pre- 
dation,  whereas  few  eggs  suggest  non-planktotrophic  de- 
velopment (Thorson,  1950;  Jablonski  and  Lutz,  1983). 
Estimates  of  fecundity  in  the  present  study  are  compli- 


cated by  the  fact  that  we  have  no  way  of  knowing  how 
many  capsules  were  produced  by  each  laying  female. 
However,  the  over  1000  larvae  contained  in  the  lenticular 
capsules  from  the  Galapagos  Rift,  when  compared  to  in- 
formation on  the  number  of  eggs  per  capsule,  total  fe- 
cundity, and  development  type  in  other  species  with  len- 
ticular egg  capsules  (Table  III),  suggests  that  this  species 
develops  planktotrophically  following  hatching.  The  ap- 
parent absence  of  nurse  eggs  and  the  presence  of  several 
hundred  embryos  in  the  inflated  triangular  capsules  from 
the  Galapagos  Rift  also  suggests  the  potential  for  plank- 
totrophic development  after  hatching;  while  the  small 
number  of  embryos  (1-6)  in  pouch-like  capsules  from  the 
Juan  de  Fuca  Ridge  is  strongly  indicative  of  non-plank- 
totrophic development. 

Non-planktotrophic  development  is  the  most  common 
strategy  among  prosobranchs  in  the  deep-sea,  soft-sedi- 
ment environment  of  the  western  North  Atlantic  (Rex 
and  Waren,  1982).  However,  an  examination  of  bathyal 
and  abyssal  prosobranch  larval  shells  reveals  that  roughly 
30%  may  develop  planktotrophically  below  1000  m  in 
the  north-eastern  Atlantic  (Bouchet,  1976a,  b;  Bouchet 
and  Waren,  1979b)  and  that  the  incidence  of  plankto- 
trophic development  in  this  group  increases  with  depth 
below  the  continental  shelf  (Rex  and  Waren,  1982).  About 
50%  of  mesogastropod  and  neogastropod  prosobranch 
species  on  the  abyssal  plain  may  have  planktotrophic  de- 
velopment based  on  their  protoconch  morphologies  (Rex 
and  Waren,  1982).  Bouchet  (1976a,  b)  and  Bouchet  and 
Waren  ( 1979b)  have  proposed  that  deep-sea  prosobranch 
larvae  with  protoconchs,  indicative  of  planktotrophy,  mi- 
grate to  feed  and  undergo  development  in  surface  waters. 
In  addition,  18O  and  I3C  isotope  analyses  of  larval  shells 
from  deep-sea  gastropods  have  suggested  that  at  least  some 
larvae  of  abyssal  species  may  migrate  upwards  into 
warmer  waters  during  development  (Bouchet  and  Fontes, 
1981;  Killingley  and  Rex,  1985). 

Protoconch  I  sculpture  of  the  type  seen  in  larvae  from 
lenticular  egg  capsules  at  the  Galapagos  Rift  (Figs.  12- 
14)  is  characteristic  of  many  turrid  species  with  putative 
planktotrophic  development  in  both  shallow  waters 
[Philhertia  linearis  (Montagu)  (Rodriguez  Babio  and 
Thiriot-Quievreux,  1974),  Ruphitoma  spp.  (Richter  and 
Thorson,  1975)]  and  in  the  deep  sea  [Pleurotomella  spp.. 
Teretia  spp.,  Xanlliodaphne  spp.,  Phymorhynchus  spp.. 
Gymnobela  spp.,  Thetu  spp.  (Bouchet  and  Waren.  1980), 
Gymnobela  sitbaraneosa  (Dautzenberg  and  Fischer) 
(Colman  et  al.,  1986)].  The  Turridae  includes  species  with 
larval  shell  morphologies  that  are  indicative  of  both 
planktotrophic  and  non-planktotrophic  development 
(Rex  and  Waren,  1982;  Shimek,  1983a,  b,  c,  1986).  The 
maximum  diameter  of  the  Protoconch  I  in  certain  plank- 
totrophic-type  turrid  species  can  be  roughly  estimated 
from  published  micrographs,  including  the  species  Phil- 


HYDROTHERMAL  VENT  EGG  CAPSULES 


hen  in  linearis  (260  nm)  (Rodriguez  Babio  and  Thiriot- 
Quievreux,  1974),  Raphitoma  reticn/ata  (Renier)  (220 
^m).  R  (P/iilhenia)  piirpurea  (Montagu)  (245  ^m).  R 
(Cirilla)  linearis  (Montagu)  (245  ^m),  R.  (Leufroyi)  leuf- 
royi  (Michaud)  (225  ^m)  (Richter  and  Thorson,  1975). 
Pleurotomella  coeloraphe  (Dautzenberg  and  Fischer)  (265 
nm),  Pleitrotomella  demosia  (Dautzenberg  and  Fischer) 
(355  ^m),  Pleurotomella  megalembryon  (Dautzenberg and 
Fischer)  (280  ^m),  Pleurotomella  hureaui  (Dautzenberg 
and  Fischer)  (220  nm),  Pleurotomella  sandersoni  Verrill 
(195  ^m),  Teretia  teres (Forbes)  (260  ^m),  Xanllunlaphne 
dalmasi  (Dautzenberg  and  Fischer)  (215  ^m),  Thela 
chariessa  (Watson)  (240  /urn)  (Bouchet  and  Waren,  1980), 
and  Gymnobela  siibaraneosa  (Dautzenberg  and  Fischer) 
(180  Mm)  (Colman  el  al.  1986). 

Assuming  that  the  larvae  in  lenticular  egg  capsules  from 
the  Galapagos  Rift  retrieved  on  "Alvin"  Dive  203 1  were 
near  hatching  and  represent  the  complete  Protoconch  I 
stage,  then  measurements  made  from  electron  micro- 
graphs reveal  that  this  species  has  a  Protoconch  I  maxi- 
mum diameter  of  approximately  235  /im  (Figs.  12-14). 
This  compares  favorably  with  the  Protoconch  I  maximum 
diameters  above,  estimated  for  various  turrid  species  ( 195- 
280  ^m),  with  the  exception  of  the  large  Protoconch  I  in 
Pleurotomella  demosia  (355  nm)  and  the  small  Proto- 
conch I  in  Gymnobela  sitbaraneosa  (180  nm).  The  max- 
imum diameter  of  the  Protoconch  I  in  larvae  from  the 
lenticular  capsules  also  agrees  well  with  the  size  of  the 
Protoconch  I  (as  estimated  from  published  micrographs) 
in  both  the  unnamed,  newly  settled  turrid  from  the  Gal- 
apagos Rift  (205-225  ^m)  and  the  unnamed  newly  settled 
turrid  from  21°N  on  the  East  Pacific  Rise  (260  /jm) 
(Turner  el  ai.  1985;  their  figs.  27a-e  and  26a-e,  respec- 
tively). Unfortunately,  pre-juvenile  development  is  in- 
complete for  larvae  in  lenticular  capsules  in  the  present 
study,  and  the  final  Protoconch  II  maximum  diameter 
and  number  of  whorls  cannot  be  determined,  which  pre- 
cludes the  estimation  of  D/Vol  values  for  these  samples. 
However,  as  estimated  from  Fig.  27a  in  Turner  el  al. 
(1985),  the  unnamed  newly  settled  turrid  from  the  Gal- 
apagos Rift  has  a  Protoconch  II  with  approximately  4 
whorls  and  a  maximum  diameter  of  950  ^m  (giving  a  D/ 
Vol  value  of  0.19).  Similarly,  the  unnamed  newly  settled 
turrid  from  21°N  (Turner  el  ai,  1985)  has  a  Protoconch 
II  maximum  diameter  of  approximately  775  ^m  with 
about  4  whorls  (giving  a  D/Vol  value  of  0.19),  as  estimated 
from  Fig.  26d  in  Turner  et  al.  (1985).  The  number  of 
volutions,  D/Vol  values,  clear  demarcation  between  Pro- 
toconch I  and  II,  and  the  protoconch  ornamentation  of 
spiral  threads  crossed  by  axial  riblets  of  both  unnamed 
newly  settled  turrids  from  the  Galapagos  Rift  and  21°N, 
depicted  in  Turner  et  al.  (1985),  are  all  indicative  of 
planktotrophic  development. 


Although  the  Protoconch  I  sculpture  of  the  unnamed 
newly  settled  turrid  from  the  Galapagos  Rift,  depicted  in 
Turner  et  al.  ( 1985.  their  figs.  27a-e),  and  Protoconch  I 
larvae  from  lenticular  egg  capsules  in  the  present  study, 
also  from  the  Galapagos  Rift  (Figs.  12-14),  cannot  be 
directly  compared  due  to  corrosion  of  the  former,  the 
similarly  sized  Protoconchs  I  (205-225  ^m  and  235  ^m, 
respectively)  suggest  that  these  specimens  are  taxonomi- 
cally  related.  Because  the  only  turrid  to  be  collected  at 
the  Galapagos  Rift  is  the  provisionally  classified  Phy- 
morhynchus  sp.  (Waren  and  Bouchet,  1989),  it  is  likely 
that  both  the  turrid  juvenile  from  the  Galapagos  Rift  de- 
scribed by  Turner  el  al.  (1985),  and  the  lenticular  egg 
capsules  and  larvae  within,  belong  to  this  species.  How- 
ever, adult  or  juvenile  Phymorhynchus  sp.  with  intact, 
non-corroded  protoconchs,  characteristics  that  would 
verify  this  identification,  have  not  been  collected  at  the 
Galapagos  Rift. 

Although  it  is  not  possible  to  determine  the  maximum 
diameter  of  the  Protoconch  I  stage  or  the  D/Vol  value  for 
larvae  from  the  inflated  triangular  egg  capsules  from  the 
Galapagos  Rift  due  to  the  incomplete  development  of  the 
larval  shell,  the  initial  reticulate  sculpture  (Figs.  19-21) 
is  indicative  of  planktotrophic  development,  following  the 
encapsulated  phase.  This  type  of  sculpture  is  similar  to 
that  seen  on  the  Protoconch  I  of  some  members  of  the 
mesogastropod  families  Rissoidae  (Lebour,  1936,  1937; 
Amio,  1963)  and  Cypraeidae  (Richter  and  Thorson, 
1975),  as  well  as  the  neogastropod  families  Columbellidae 
(Colman  el  al.,  1986)  and  Turridae  (Lebour,  1934;  Amio, 
1963;  Rodriguez  Babio  and  Thiriot-Quievreux,  1974; 
Richter  and  Thorson,  1975;  Bouchet,  1976a;  Bouchet  and 
Waren.  1980).  Members  of  the  Cypraeidae  have  not  been 
collected  in  the  deep-sea  (Clarke,  1962),  and  no  member 
of  this  family  or  of  the  Rissoidae  has  been  collected  at  the 
Galapagos  Rift.  The  columbellid  Anachis  haliaeeti  (Jef- 
freys) from  the  Rockall  Trough  has  an  ornately  sculptured 
Protoconch  I  and  has  been  designated  a  planktotrophic 
developer  (Colman  el  al.,  1986).  Shallow-water  species  of 
this  genus  produce  small  egg  capsules  similar  to  the  in- 
flated triangular  type  from  the  Galapagos  Rift  but  with  a 
circular  collar  around  the  capsule's  apex  and  containing 
only  10-30  embryos  (Scheltema,  1969)  in  contrast  to  the 
several  hundred  larvae  seen  in  capsules  from  the  Gala- 
pagos Rift.  Likewise,  the  morphology  of  the  inflated  tri- 
angular egg  capsule  is  unlike  that  seen  in  most  Turridae. 
However,  the  egg  capsule  attributed  to  the  turrid  Oenopota 
ovalis •( Bouchet  and  Waren,  1979a)  resembles  the  inflated 
triangular  capsules,  but  contains  only  one  embryo  with 
an  unsculptured  protoconch,  in  contrast  to  the  several 
hundred  highly  ornamented  larvae  encountered  in  the 
inflated  triangular  capsules  from  the  Galapagos  Rift. 
Given  the  incomplete  formation  of  the  larval  shell  and 
unique  structure  of  the  egg  capsule,  it  has  proved  impos- 


50 


R.  G.  GUSTAFSON  ET  AL. 


sible  to  assign  the  inflated  triangular  capsules  from  the 
Galapagos  Rift  to  any  known  gastropod  species  from  this 
site. 

Larvae  from  the  Juan  de  Fuca  Ridge  egg  capsules  have 
a  large,  paucispiral  protoconch  devoid  of  sculpture  (Figs. 
25,  26),  which  suggests  that  this  species  develops  non- 
planktotrophically.  Assuming  that  these  larvae  have  a 
nearly  fully  developed  protoconch,  with  a  maximum  di- 
ameter of  approximately  1.3  mm  and  1.5  whorls  (as  es- 
timate from  Fig.  26),  their  calculated  D/Vol  value  of  0.87 
in  concert  with  the  small  number  of  whorls  is  also  indic- 
ative of  non-planktotrophic  development.  This  type  of 
protoconch,  lacking  ornamentation,  is  similar  to  that 
produced  by  members  of  the  neogastropod  superfamilies 
Muricoidea  and  Cancellaroidea  (Thorson,  1935;  Radwin 
and  Chamberlin,  1973:  Bandel.  1975a,  b,  c;  Bouchet  and 
Waren,  1985a;  Colman  et  a/.,  1986;  Colman  and  Tyler, 
1988),  however  taxonomic  placement  of  these  larvae  is 
uncertain  using  protoconch  morphology  alone.  The  pro- 
toconch of  the  cancellaroid  Admete  viridu/a  (Thorson, 
1935:  fig.  72),  the  species  with  the  most  similar  egg  capsule 
morphology  to  the  specimens  described  in  this  study  from 
the  Juan  de  Fuca  Ridge,  has  a  maximum  diameter  of 
about  0.88  mm  [as  estimated  from  Thorson  (1935:  fig. 
72)]  and  1.25  whorls,  for  a  D/Vol  value  of  0.77,  which 
also  compares  favorably  with  that  calculated  for  encap- 
sulated larvae  from  the  Juan  de  Fuca  Ridge  (0.87). 
Bouchet  and  Waren  (1985a)  give  the  protoconch  maxi- 
mum diameters  and  number  of  whorls  for  the  deep-sea 
buccinids  Eosipho  thorybopux  Bouchet  and  Waren  (0.7 
mm,  1  whorl,  D/Vol  =  0.7),  Manaria  lirata  Kuroda  and 
Habe  (0.8  mm,  1+  whorl,  D/Vol  =  0.8),  and  M.  clan- 
destina  Bouchet  and  Waren  (0.7  mm,  1+  whorl,  D/Vol 
=  0.7).  Colman  et  al.  (1986)  also  provide  protoconch 
maximum  diameters,  whorl  number,  and  D/Vol  values 
for  the  deep-sea  buccinids  Tacit  a  abyssorum  (Locard) 
(0.75  mm,  1.2  whorls,  D/Vol  =  0.63),  Coins  jeffreysianus 
(Fischer)  (2.5  mm,  1.25  whorls,  D/Vol  =  2),  and  the 
muricid  Trophon  sp.  ( 1 . 1  mm,  1 .5  whorls,  D/Vol  =  0.73). 
All  of  these  species  lack  protoconch  ornamentation.  Or- 
namentation is  also  lacking  on  some  members  of  the  me- 
sogastropod  families  Rissoidae  (Richter  and  Thorson, 
1975)  and  Cerithiidae  (Rodriguez  Babio  and  Thiriot- 
Quievreux,  1974),  as  well  as  the  neogastropod  families 
Nassariidae  (Richter  and  Thorson,  1975)  and  Turridae 
(Bouchet  and  Waren.  1980;  Colman  et  al.,  1986). 

Protoconchs  retained  on  adult  specimens  of  the  two 
known  gastropod  species  from  the  Juan  de  Fuca  Ridge, 
large  enough  to  have  laid  the  capsules  from  this  site  [Buc- 
cinum  viridum  (pers.  obs.)  and  Provanna  variabilis(  Waren 
and  Bouchet,  1986)],  were  badly  corroded,  precluding 
comparison  with  larvae  extracted  from  the  Juan  de  Fuca 
Ridge  egg  capsules.  The  potential  for  either  of  these  two 


species  to  be  the  source  of  the  pouch-like  capsules  from 
the  Juan  de  Fuca  Ridge  is  discussed  above. 

Molluscan  larval  dispersal  at  hydrothermal  vents 

Despite  the  ephemeral  nature  and  patchy  distribution 
of  hydrothermal  vent  environments,  an  analysis  of  the 
developmental  mode  of  30  species  of  mollusks  (gastropods 
and  bivalves)  present  at  three  deep-sea  hydrothermal  vents 
(13°N,  21  °N  and  Galapagos  Rift;  Lutz  el  al..  1980.  1984, 
1986;  Turner  and  Lutz,  1984;  Turner  et  al.,  1985;  Lutz, 
1988)  suggested  that  only  three  species  (two  turrids  and 
a  mytilid)  have  larvae  capable  of  long-range  dispersal.  The 
other  27  species  have  larval  shell  morphologies  indicative 
of  non-planktotrophic,  low-dispersal  modes  of  develop- 
ment. These  developmental  patterns  are  all  typical  of  the 
shallow-water  members  of  the  systematic  group  to  which 
these  vent  species  belong.  Although  some  unusual  ad- 
aptations may  be  present  among  larvae  of  vent  organisms, 
such  as  prolonged  delay  of  metamorphosis  in  response  to 
low  deep-sea  temperatures  (Lutz  et  al.,  1980,  1984,  1986; 
Turner  and  Lutz,  1984;  Turner  et  al..  1985;  Lutz,  1988), 
inferences  made  from  egg  size,  fecundity,  and  larval  mor- 
phology suggest  that  unique  adaptations  to  ensure  suc- 
cessful larval  dispersal  between  vent  habitats  have  not 
evolved  in  hydrothermal  vent  mollusks  (Turner  et  al., 
1985;  Waren  and  Bouchet,  1989).  However,  many  of  the 
molluscan  morpho-species  described  from  vents  in  the 
eastern  Pacific  are  present  at  more  than  one  vent  field  (9 
are  shared  by  Galapagos  Rift  and  13°N;  10  are  shared  by 
Galapagos  Rift  and  21°N;  18  are  shared  by  13°N  and 
2 1  °N;  2  are  shared  by  Juan  de  Fuca  and  Explorer  Ridges; 
and  7  are  shared  by  Galapagos  Rift,  13°N  and  21°N) 
(Boss  and  Turner,  1980:  Kenk  and  Wilson,  1985;Schein- 
Fatton,  1985:  McLean  and  Haszprunar,  1987;  McLean, 
1988.  1989a.  b:  Waren  and  Bouchet,  1986,  1989).  This 
paradox  may  be  partially  explained  by  recent  findings  of 
vent  fauna  on  or  near  whale  carcasses  (Smith  et  al.,  1989; 
but  see  Tunnicliffe  and  Juniper,  1990)  and  at  cold  meth- 
ane and  sulfide  seeps  (Paull  et  al.,  1984;  Kennicutt  et  al., 
1985,  1989;  Juniper  and  Sibuet,  1987;  Mayer  et  al..  1988), 
which  may  serve  as  stepping-stone  habitats  for  dispersal 
between  vents.  In  addition,  Johannesson  (1988),  and  to 
a  lesser  degree  others  (Palmer  and  Strathmann,  1981; 
Burton,  1983;  Highsmith,  1985;  Hedgecock,  1986;  Jack- 
son, 1986;  R.  R.  Strathmann,  1987;  Safriel  and  Hadfield, 
1988;  O'Foighil,  1989),  question  the  effective  dispersal 
benefits  of  the  planktotrophic  versus  the  non-plankto- 
trophic mode  of  development,  over  long  distances.  It  is 
suggested  that  a  small  founder  group  of  direct  developers 
or  hermaphroditic  individuals,  passively  transported  to  a 
new  site  as  adults  or  in  a  drifted  egg  mass,  would  have  an 
advantage  over  planktonic  developers  in  establishing  a 
new  colony,  because  their  offspring  would  remain  in  the 


HYDROTHERMAL  VENT  EGG  CAPSULES 


51 


immediate  vicinity  of  the  founder  group  where  the  en- 
counter rate  with  mates  would  be  high.  On  the  other  hand, 
the  offspring  of  planktonic  developers  are  free-swimming 
for  weeks  and  may  settle  far  from  the  founder  group,  and 
from  each  other,  where  the  encounter  rate  with  mates  is 
low  (Johannesson.  1988). 

Similar,  if  not  identical,  species  of  Phymorhynchus  oc- 
cur at  the  Galapagos  Rift  and  at  1 3°N  and  2 1  °N  on  the 
East  Pacific  Rise  (Waren  and  Bouchet,  1989).  The  plank- 
totrophic-type  larvae  found  in  turrid  egg  capsules  from 
the  Galapagos  Rift,  putatively  identified  as  the  spawn  of 
Phymorhynchus  sp.,  suggests  that  this  species  has  a  great 
potential  for  disperal  and  may  explain  its  apparent  wide 
distribution  (but  see  discussion  above).  Shimek  (I983a, 
b,  1986)  has  cultured  three  shallow-water  turrids, 
Ophiodermclla  imrmis  (Hinds).  Ocnopolu  levidensis 
(Carpenter),  and  Oenopota  elegans  (Moller).  with  encap- 
sulated periods  of  50  days,  50  days,  and  42-49  days,  re- 
spectively, and  free-swimming  periods  of  35  days.  7-10 
days,  and  42-49  days,  respectively.  In  addition,  Oenopota 
levidensis  assumes  a  benthic  existence  and  develops  for  a 
further  25  days  prior  to  metamorphosis  as  a  "demersal- 
planktotrophic  larva,"  using  the  terminology  of  Shimek 
(1986).  If  parallel  conditions  obtain  in  deep-sea  turrids 
the  potential  for  dispersal  of  larvae  during  a  1-7  week 
planktonic  phase,  given  known  bottom  currents  on  the 
East  Pacific  Rise  (Lonsdale,  1977),  would  be  on  the  order 
of  hundreds  of  kilometers  (Lutz  et  a/..  1980).  A  maximum 
current  speed  of  18  cm  s~'  was  recorded  at  a  site  50  m 
above  the  crest  of  the  East  Pacific  Rise  within  350  m  of 
a  suspected  hydrothermal  plume  (Lonsdale,  1977).  In  ad- 
dition, a  decrease  in  developmental  rate  in  response  to 
cold  ambient  bottom  waters  away  from  the  vents  may 
increase  the  length  of  larval  life  and  further  enhance  dis- 
peral (Lutz  et  ai,  1980,  1984,  1986;  Turner  and  Lutz, 
1984;  Turner  et  ai.  1985;  Lutz,  1988).  Based  on  com- 
parison of  the  stable  isotope  compositions  (18O:'6O  ratios) 
of  adult  and  larval  shells,  Killingley  and  Rex  (1985)  re- 
ported that  three  deep-sea  turrids  with  similar  protoconch 
sculpture  to  that  seen  in  Phymorhynchus  (Theta  lyronu- 
clea,  T.  chariessa  and  Pleurotomella  sandersoni ),  migrate 
vertically,  as  larvae,  to  develop  in  warm  surface  waters. 
At  present,  it  is  indeterminable  whether  Phymorhynchus 
larvae  complete  their  development  as  demersal  feeders, 
ascend  to  feed  on  plankton  in  surface  waters,  or  undergo 
some  combination  of  these  developmental  modes.  The 
fact  that  Phymorhynchus  is  either  a  predator  or  scavenger 
at  the  vents  (Waren  and  Bouchet,  1989)  indicates  that  it 
may  not  be  restricted  to  vent  habitats,  although  it  is  able 
to  tolerate  the  extreme  vent  environment  and  exploit  the 
abundant  food  energy  available  at  these  sites.  Adult  Phy- 
morhynchus are  mobile  and  the  extent  to  which  move- 
ments of  adults  aid  in  dispersal  is  unknown. 


Although  it  has  not  been  possible  to  unambiguously 
identify  the  species  to  which  the  inflated  triangular  egg 
capsules  from  the  Galapagos  Rift  or  the  pouch-like  egg 
capsules  from  the  Juan  de  Fuca  Ridge  belong,  we  can 
infer  something  about  the  mode  of  development  of  these 
two  organisms.  The  presence  of  several  hundred  veliger 
larvae  in  inflated  triangular  capsules  from  the  Galapagos 
Rift  and  the  intricate  sculpture  on  the  early  protoconch 
both  suggest  that  this  species  may  develop  planktotroph- 
ically.  On  the  other  hand,  the  small  number  of  larvae  (1- 
6)  in  capsules  from  the  Juan  de  Fuca  Ridge  and  the  pro- 
toconch's  large  size,  inferred  value  of  D/Vol,  and  un- 
sculptured  appearance  all  suggest  that  this  species  has  a 
non-planktotrophic  mode  of  development. 

How  relatively  sedentary  organisms  at  deep-sea  hydro- 
thermal  vents  locate  and  colonize  these  geographically 
isolated  environments  remains  an  open  question.  With 
the  exception  of  a  few  preliminary  population  genetic 
studies  (J.  P.  Grassle,  1985;  Bucklin,  1988).  our  knowledge 
of  colonization,  gene  flow  and  dispersal  of  organisms  be- 
tween hydrothermal  vents  has  been  obtained  from  infer- 
ences drawn  from  egg  capsule  type,  egg  size,  fecundity, 
and  larval  morphologies  retained  on  adults  (Lutz,  1988). 
Further  zoogeographic  data  and  systematic  descriptions 
are  needed  before  we  can  provide  more  rigorous  answers 
to  questions  involving  the  mechanisms  of  dispersal  and 
rates  of  gene  flow  between  isolated  areas  of  deep-sea  hy- 
drothermal vent  activity.  Laboratory  culture  of  these  un- 
usual deep-sea  molluscan  taxa  is  also  necessary  to  confirm 
the  link  between  larval  shell  characteristics  and  the  mode 
of  development.  If  we  assume  that  the  majority  of  the 
vent  fauna  is  endemic  to  the  hydrothermal  vent  habitat 
[(but  see  contrasting  opinion  of  Clarke  (1986)],  and  that 
larval  dispersal  in  non-planktotrophic  species  is  a  step- 
wise  process,  then  each  ridge  axis  should  be  a  discrete 
dispersal  corridor.  Given  these  assumptions,  genetic  re- 
latedness  of  the  most  widely  separated  non-planktotrophic 
species'  populations  along  a  single  ridge  axis  should  be 
more  homogeneous  than  among  populations  that  are 
equally  separated  but  belong  to  two  different  ridge  axes. 
On  the  other  hand,  genetic  relatedness  of  species  with 
planktotrophic  development  should  be  more  homogenous 
in  the  prevailing  direction  of  bottom  currents  and  less 
reliant  on  the  configuration  of  ridge  systems.  The  studies 
of  J.  P.  Grassle  (1985)  and  Bucklin  (1988)  provide  highly 
paradoxical  results.  In  Bathymodiolus  thermophilus,  a 
species  reported  to  have  a  lengthy  dispersal  stage  (Lutz  et 
ai,  1980),  populations  from  the  Galapagos  Rift  and  13°N 
(separated  by  2200  km)  are  genetically  distinct.  Yet  in 
Riftiapachyptila,  a  non-molluscan  species  that  is  believed 
to  have  lecithotrophic,  demersal  larvae  with  limited  dis- 
persal abilities,  more  widely  separated  populations  from 
the  Galapagos  Rift  and  2 1  °N  along  the  East  Pacific  Rise 
(separated  by  3300  km)  are  genetically  similar  (Bucklin, 


52 


R.  G.  GUSTAFSON  ET  .-)/_ 


1988).  Clearly,  an  expanded  research  effort  using  electro- 
phoretic  and  molecular  techniques  to  ascertain  population 
structure  within  species  and  genetic  relatedness  among 
species,  coupled  with  analyses  of  molluscan  larval  shell 
morphology,  will  be  needed  to  answer  questions  concern- 
ing the  rates  of  gene  flow  between  discrete  areas  of  hy- 
drothermal  activity  associated  with  contiguous  and  non- 
contiguous oceanic  ridge  systems,  as  well  as  the  validity 
of  using  larval  shell  morphology  to  ascertain  dispersal  ca- 
pability in  the  deep  sea. 

Acknowledgments 

We  thank  the  pilots  and  crew  of  "Atlantis  II"/"Alvin" 
for  invaluable  technical  assistance  with  the  retrieval  of 
specimens;  Robert  Hessler  of  Scripps  Institution  of 
Oceanography  for  his  extraordinary  powers  of  observation 
which  led  to  retrieval  of  marker  H9  and  its  attached  egg 
capsules  from  the  Galapagos  Rift;  John  Grazul  of  the 
Electron  Microscopy  Facility,  Nelson  Biological  Labo- 
ratories, Rutgers  University,  for  assistance  with  SEM; 
Meridith  L.  Jones  of  the  Smithsonian  Institution,  for 
samples  from  Juan  de  Fuca  Ridge;  and  Lowell  Fritz  for 
critical  comments  throughout  this  study  and  for  critically 
reading  early  versions  of  the  manuscript.  This  is  Contri- 
bution #90-30  of  the  Institute  of  Marine  and  Coastal  Sci- 
ences, Rutgers  University  and  New  Jersey  Agricultural 
Experiment  Station  Publication  No.  D-32402-1-90,  sup- 
ported by  State  funds  and  by  NSF  Grants  OCE-87- 16591 
and  OCE-89- 17311. 

Literature  Cited 

Amio,  M.  1955.  On  the  eggs  and  early  life  histories  of  Pyrenidae  (Col- 
umbellidae)  in  marine  gastropods.  J.  Shimonoseki  Co/I.  Fish.  4:  231- 
238. 

Amio,  M.  1963.  A  comparative  embryology  of  marine  gastropods,  with 
ecological  considerations.  J.  Shimonoseki  Coll.  Fish.  12:  229-358. 

Anderson,  D.  T.  1960.  The  life  histories  of  marine  prosobranch  gas- 
tropods. J.  Malawi.  Soc.  Ansl.  4:  16-29. 

Anderson,  D.  T.  1962.  The  reproduction  and  early  life  histories  ot  the 
gastropods  Bembicium  aiirantin  (Quoy  and  Gaimard)  (Fam.  Litto- 
rinidae).  Ccllana  tramoserica  (Sower.)  (Fam.  Patellidae)  and  Mela- 
nerita  melanotragus  (Smith)  (Fam.  Neritidae).  Proc.  Linn.  Soc.  N. 
S.  ir.  87:62-68. 

Anderson,  D.  T.  1965.  Further  observations  on  the  life  histories  of 
littoral  gastropods  in  New  South  Wales.  Proc.  Linn.  Soc.  N.  S.  W. 
90:242-251. 

Arnaud,  P.  M.,  and  H.  Zibrowius.  1973.  Capsules  ovigeres  de  gastero- 
podes  Turridae  et  corrosion  du  squelette  des  scleractinaires  bathyaux 
des  Acores.  Rev.  Fac.  Cienc.  Univ.  Lisb.  Ser.  C  Cienc  Nat.  17:  581- 
597. 

Bacci,  G.  1947.  Le  capsule  ovigere  di  Columbella  ntslica  (L.)  e  di  /•'<«- 
ciolaria  lignaria  (L.)  (Prosobranchia.  Stenoglossa).  Boll.  Zool.  14: 
75-81. 

Bandel,  K.  1974a.  Spawning  and  development  of  some  Columbellidae 
from  the  Caribbean  Sea  of  Colombia  (South  America).  I'eliger  16: 
271-282. 


Bandel,  K.  1974b.     Studies  on  Littorinidae  from  the  Atlantic.  I'eliger 

17:92-114. 
Bandel,  K.  1975a.     Embryonalgehause  karibischer  Meso-  und  Neogas- 

tropoden  (Mollusca).  Abhandlungen  der  Mathematisch-Naturwis- 

\cnschajilichen  Masse.  Akademic  tier  Wissenshaften  und  tier  Liter- 
aim:  Maim.  Jahrgang  IV7\  A>  /..  133  pp. 
Bandel.  K.  1975b.  Embryonale  und  larvale  Schale  einiger  Prosobranchier 

(Gastropoda,  Mollusca)  der  Oosterschelde  (Nordsee).  Hydrohiol.  Bull 

9:  3-22. 
Bandel,  K.  1975c.     Das  Embryonalgehause  mariner  Prosobranchier  der 

Region  von  Banyuls-sur-Mer.  I  7c  Milieu  Ser.  A  Biol.  Mar.  25:  83- 

1  18. 
Bandel,  K.  1976a.     Observations  on  spawn,  embryonic  development 

and  ecology  of  some  Caribbean  lower  Mesogastropoda.  Veliger  18: 

249-270. 
Bandel,  K.  1976b.     Spawning,  development  and  ecology  of  some  higher 

Neogastropoda  from  the  Caribbean  Sea  of  Colombia  (South  America). 

Veliger  19:  176-193. 
Bandel,  K.  1982.     Morphologic  und  Bildung  der  friihontogenetischen 

Gehause  bei  conchiferan  Mollusken.  Fades  1:  1-198. 
Berg,  C.  J.,  Jr.  1985.     Reproductive  strategies  of  mollusks  from  abyssal 

hydrothermal  vent  communities.  In  The  Hydrothermal  Vents  of  the 

Eastern  Pacific:  An  Overview.  M.  L.  Jones,  ed.  Biol.  Soc.  ll'ash.  Bull. 

6:  185-197. 
Boss,  K.  J.,  and  R.  D.  Turner.  1980.     The  giant  white  clam  from  the 

Galapagos  Rift.  Calyptogena  magnitica.  species  novum.  Ma/acologia 

20:  161-194. 
Bouchel,  P.  1976a.     Mise  en  evidence  de  stades  larvaires  planctoniques 

chez  des  Gasteropodes  Prosobranches  des  etages  bathyal  et  abyssal. 

Bull.  Mits.  Nail.  Hist.  Nat.  Zool.  277:  947-971. 
Bouchet,  P.  1976b.     Mise  en  evidence  d'une  migration  de  larves  veligeres 

entre  I'etage  abyssal  et  la  surface.  C.  R.  Hebd.  Seances  Acad.  Sd. 

Ser.  D  Set.  Nal.  283:  821-824. 
Bouchel,  P.  1981.     Evolution  of  larval  development  in  Eastern  Atlantic 

Terebridae  (Gastropoda),  Neogene  to  Recent.  Malacologia  21:  363- 

369. 
Bouchet,  P.  1989.     A  review  of  poecilogony  in  gastropods.  J.  Moll.  Stud. 

55:  67-78. 
Bouchet,  P.,  and  J-C.  Fontes.  1981.     Migrations  verticales  des  larves  de 

Gasteropodes  abyssaux:  arguments  nouveaux  dus  a  1'analyse  isoto- 

pique  de  la  coquille  larvaire  et  postlarvaire.  C  R   Hebd.  Seances 

Acad.  Sd.  Ser.  Ill  292:  1005-1008. 
Bouchet,  P.,  and  A.  \\aren.  I979a.     The  abysal  molluscan  fauna  of  the 

Norwegian  Sea  and  its  relation  to  other  faunas.  Sarsia  64:  21 1-243. 
Bouchet,  P.,  and  A.  \\aren.  1979b.     Planktotrophic  larval  development 

in  deep-water  gastropods.  Sarsia  64:  37-40. 
Bouchet,  P.,  and  A.  \\aren.  1980.     Revision  of  the  north-east  Atlantic 

bathyal  and  abyssal  Turndae  (Mollusca,  Gastropoda).  J.  Moll.  Stud 

Suppl.  8:  1-119. 
Bouchet,  P.,  and  A.  \\aren.  1985a.     Mollusca  Gastropoda:  Taxonomical 

notes  on  tropical  deep  water  Buccinidae  with  descriptions  of  new 

taxa.  Mem.  Mus.  Nail.  Hist  Nat.  Ser.  A  Zool.  133:  457-499. 
Bouchet,  P.,  and  A.  Waren.  I985b.     Revision  of  the  north-east  Atlantic 

bathyal  and  abyssal  Neogastropoda,  excluding  Tumdae.  Boll.  Ma- 

lacol.  Suppl.  1:  121-296. 
Hiii-Hin.  A.  1988.     Allozymic  variability  of  Riftia  pachypnla  populations 

from  the  Galapagos  Rift  and  21°N  hydrothermal  vents.  Deep-Sea 

Res  35:  1759-1768. 
Burton,  R.  S.  1983.     Protein  polymorphisms  and  genetic  differentiation 

of  marine  invertebrate  populations.  Mar.  Biol.  Lett.  4:  193-206. 
Clarke,  A.  H.  1962.     Annotated  list  and  bibliography  of  the  abyssal 

marine  molluscs  of  the  world.  Bull.  Nail.  Mils.  Canada  181:  1-1 14. 
Clarke,  A.  H.  1986.     Some  alternative  views  about  deep-sea  mollusks 

from  the  Arctic  and  from  hydrothermal  vents.  Malacology  Data  Net 

1:49-57. 


HYDROTHERMAL  VENT  EGG  CAPSULES 


Colman,  J.  G.,  and  P.  A.  Tyler.  1988.  The  egg  capsules  of  ( 'O/H.V  /(•/• 
freysianus  (Fischer,  1868)  (Prosobranchia:  Neogastropoda)  from  the 
Rockall  Trough,  Nonh  East  Atlantic.  Sarsia  73:  139-145. 

Colman,  J.  G.,  P.  A.  Tyler,  and  J.  D.  Gage.  1986.  Larval  development 
of  deep-sea  gastropods  (Prosobranchia:  Neogastropoda)  from  the 
Rockall  Trough.  /  Mar.  Biol.  Assoc.  IK  66:  951-965. 

Cowan,  I.  M.  196-4.  The  egg  capsule  and  young  of  Beringius  everdanu 
Smith.  I  'cli.wr  1:  43-44. 

Crane,  K.  1985.  The  distribution  of  geothermal  fields  along  the  mid- 
ocean  ridge:  an  overview.  In  The  Hydrothermal  I  'ems  of  tin-  Eastern 
Pacific  An  Ovcn'ic»;  M.  L.  Jones,  ed.  Biol.  Sue.  II  'ash  Bull  6:  3- 
18. 

Crisp,  D.  J.  1978.  Genetic  consequences  of  different  reproductive 
strategies  in  marine  invertebrates.  Pp.  257-273  in  Marine  Organisms: 
Genetics.  Ecology,  and  Evolution.  B.  Battaglia  and  J.  A.  Beardmore, 
eds.  Plenum  Press,  New  York. 

Dall,  \V.  H.  1890.  Scientific  results  of  explorations  by  the  U.S.  Fish 
Commission  steamer  "Albatross."  VII.  Preliminary  report  on  the 
collection  of  Mollusca  and  Brachiopoda.  Proi:  US  Nail.  Mus  12: 
219-362. 

Dall,  \V.  H.  192-4.  On  the  value  of  nuclear  characters  in  the  classification 
of  marine  gastropods.  J  \Vash.Acad  Sci  1-4:  177-180. 

D'Asaro,  C.  N.  1970a.  Egg  capsules  of  prosobranch  mollusks  from 
South  Florida  and  the  Bahamas  and  notes  on  spawning  in  the  lab- 
oratory. Bull.  Mar  Sci.  20:  414-440. 

D'Asaro,  C.  N.  1970b.  Egg  capsules  of  some  prosobranchs  from  the 
Pacific  coast  of  Panama.  I'cliger  13:  37-43. 

D'Asaro,  C.  N.  1986.  Egg  capsules  of  eleven  marine  prosobranchs  from 
northwest  Florida.  Bull  Afar.  Sci.  39:  76-9 1 . 

D'Asaro,  C.  N.  1988.  Micromorphology  of  neogastropod  egg  capsules. 
Nautilus  102:  134-148. 

Eyster,  L.  S.,  and  M.  P.  Morse.  1984.  Early  shell  formation  during 
molluscan  embryogenesis,  with  new  studies  on  the  surf  clam,  Spisula 
soluhssima  Am.  7.ool.  24:  871-882. 

Franc,  A.  1950.  Ponte  et  larves  planctoniques  de  Philbcrtia  purpurea 
(Montagu).  Bull  Lab  Maril.  Dinard 33:  23-25. 

Fretter,  V.  1984.  Prosobranchs.  Pp.  1-45  in  The  Mollusca.  Vol.  7. 
Reproduction.  A.  S.  Tompa,  N.  H.  Verdonk  and  J.  A.  M.  van  den 
Biggelaar.  eds.  Academic  Press.  New  York. 

Fretter,  \  .,  and  A.  Graham.  1962.  British  Prosobranch  .\to/liisc\  I  heir 
Functional  Anatomy  and  Ecology.  Bartholomew  Press.  London.  755 
pp. 

Golikov,  A.  N.  1961.  Ecology  of  reproduction  and  the  nature  of  egg 
capsules  in  some  gastropod  molluscs  of  the  genus  Neptimca  (Bolten). 
Zoo/.  Z/i.  40:997-1008. 

Grassle,  J.  F.  1985.  Hydrothermal  vent  animals:  distribution  and  bi- 
ology. Science  229:  713-725. 

Grassle,  J.  F.  1986.  The  ecology  of  deep-sea  hydrothermal  vent  com- 
munities. Adv.  Mar  Biol.  23:  301-362. 

Grassle,  J.  P.  1985.  Genetic  differentiation  in  populations  of  hydro- 
thermal  vent  mussels  (Bathymodiolus  thermophilus)  from  the  Gal- 
apagos Rift  and  1 3°N  on  the  East  Pacific  Rise.  In  The  Hydrothermal 
I  'ems  of  the  Eastern  Pacific:  An  Overview.  M.  L.  Jones,  ed.  Biol.  Soc. 
Wash.  Bull  6:429-442. 

llabe,  I.  1960.  Egg  masses  and  egg  capsules  of  some  Japanese  marine 
prosobranchiate  gastropod.  Bull  Mar  Biol.  Station  Asamushi  10: 
121-126. 

Hadfield,  M.  G.,  and  M.  F.  Strathmann.  1990.  Heterostrophic  shells 
and  pelagic  development  in  trochoideans:  implications  for  classifi- 
cation, phylogeny  and  palaeoecology.  /  Moll.  Stud.  56:  239-256. 

Hansen,  T.  A.  1982.  Modes  of  larval  development  in  Early  Tertiary 
neogastropods.  Paleobiology  8:  367-377. 

Hansen,  T.  A.  1983.  Modes  of  larval  development  and  rates  of  specia- 
tion  in  Early  Tertiary  neogastropods.  Science  220:  501-502. 


Hedgecock,  D.  1986.  Is  gene  flow  from  pelagic  larval  dispersal  important 
in  the  adaptation  and  evolution  of  marine  invertebrates?  Bull.  Mar. 
Sci.  39:  550-564. 

Heslinga,  G.  A.  1981.  Larval  development,  settlement  and  metamor- 
phosis of  the  tropical  gastropod  Troclnis  niloticus.  Malacologia  20: 
349-357. 

Highsmith,  R.  C.  1985.  Floating  and  algal  rafting  as  potential  dispersal 
mechanisms  in  brooding  invertebrates.  Mar.  Ecol.  Prog.  Ser.  25:  169- 
179. 

Hines,  A.  H.  1986.  Larval  problems  and  perspectives  in  life  histories 
of  marine  invertebrates.  Bull.  Mar.  Sci.  39:  506-525. 

Hoagland,  K.  E.,  and  R.  Robertson.  1988.  An  assessment  of  poecilogony 
in  marine  invertebrates:  phenomenon  or  fantasy?  Biol.  Bull.  174: 
109-125. 

llyman,  L.  H.  1967.  The  Invertebrates,  ^'ol  1 7.  Mollusca  I  McGraw- 
Hill,  New  York.  790  pp. 

Jablonski.  D.  1985.  Molluscan  development.  Pp.  33-49  in  Mollusks. 
Notes  For  a  Shon  Course.  T.  W.  Broadhead.  ed.  LJniversity  of  Ten- 
nessee Studies  in  Geology  13,  University  of  Tennessee  Press,  Knox- 
ville. 

Jablonski,  D.  1986.  Larval  ecology  and  macroevolution  in  marine  in- 
vertebrates. Bull.  Mar  Sci.  39:  565-587. 

Jablonski.  D.,  and  R.  A.  Lutz.  1980.  Molluscan  larval  shell  morphology: 
ecological  and  paleontological  applications.  Pp.  323-377  in  Skeletal 
Growth  of  Aquatic  Organisms.  D  C.  Rhoads  and  R.  A.  Lutz,  eds. 
Plenum  Press,  New  York. 

Jablonski,  D.,  and  R.  A.  l.utz.  1983.  Larval  ecology  of  marine  benthic 
invertebrates:  paleobiological  implications.  Biol.  Rev  58:  21-89. 

Jackson,  J.  B.  C.  1986.  Modes  ot  dispersal  of  clonal  benthic  inverte- 
brates: consequences  for  species'  distributions  and  genetic  structure 
of  local  populations.  Bull.  Mai:  Sci.  39:  588-606. 

Johannesson,  K.  1988.  The  paradox  of  Rockall:  why  is  a  brooding 
gastropod  (Lillorina  sa.\alilis)  more  widespread  than  one  having  a 
planktonic  dispersal  stage  (L.  litloreaY!  Mar  Biol  99:  507-513. 

Jung,  P.  1975.  Quaternary  larval  gastropods  from  leg  15.  site  147,  deep 
sea  drilling  project.  Preliminary  report.  I'eliger  18:  109-126. 

Juniper,  S.  K.,  and  M.  Sibuet.  1987.  Cold  seep  benthic  communities 
in  Japan  subduction  zones:  spatial  organization,  trophic  strategies 
and  evidence  for  temporal  evolution.  Mar.  Ecol.  Prog.  Ser.  40:  1 15- 
126. 

Kenk,  V.  C.,  and  B.  R.  Wilson.  1985.  A  new  mussel  (Bivalvia.  Mytilidae) 
from  hydrothermal  vents  in  the  Galapagos  Rift  zone.  Malacologia 
26:253-271. 

Kennicutt,  M.  C.,  II,  J.  M.  Brooks,  R.  R.  Bidigare,  T.  L.  Wade,  and 
R.  R.  Faye.  1985.  Vent  type  taxa  in  a  hydrocarbon  seep  region  on 
the  Louisiana  slope.  Nature  317:  351-353. 

Kennicutt,  M.  C.,  II,  J.  M.  Brooks,  R.  R.  Bidigare,  S.  J.  McDonald,  and 
D.  L.  Adkison.  1989.  An  upper  slope  "cold"  seep  community: 
northern  California.  Limnol.  Oceanogr.  34:  635-640. 

Kesteven,  H.  L.  1912.  The  constitution  of  the  gastropod  protoconch: 
its  value  as  a  taxonomic  feature  and  the  significance  of  some  of  its 
forms.  Proc.  Linn.  Soc.  New  South  M  'ales  37:  49-82. 

killingley,  J.  S.,  and  M.  A.  Rex.  1985.  Mode  of  larval  development 
in  some  deep-sea  gastropods  indicated  by  oxygen- 1 8  values  of  their 
carbonate  shells.  Deep-Sea  Res.  32:  809-818. 

kohn.  A.  J.  1961.  Studies  on  spawning  behavior,  egg  masses,  and  larval 
development  in  the  gastropod  genus  Comix.  II.  Observations  in  the 
Indian  Ocean  during  the  Yale  Seychelles  Expedition.  Bull.  Bingham 
Oceanogr.  Collect.  Yale  Univ.  17:  3-51. 

Lebour,  M.  V.  1934.  The  eggs  and  larvae  of  some  British  Turridae.  J. 
Mar.  Biol.  Assoc.  U.K.  19:  541-554. 

Lebour,  M.  V.  1936.  Notes  on  the  eggs  and  larvae  of  some  Plymouth 
prosobranchs.  /  Mar.  Biol.  Assoc.  U.K  20:  547-564. 


54 


R.  G.  GUSTAFSON  ET  AL. 


Lebour,  M.  \r.  1937.  The  eggs  and  larvae  of  the  British  prosobranchs 
with  special  reference  to  those  living  in  the  plankton.  J  Mar  Biol. 
Assoc  i'.K  22:  105-166. 

Lima,  G.  M.,  and  R.  A.  Lutz.  1990.  The  relationship  of  larval  shell 
morphology  to  mode  of  development  in  marine  prosohranch  gastro- 
pods. J  Mar.  Bid/  Assoc  I  A  70:  61  1-637. 

Lonsdale,  P.  1977.  Clustering  of  suspension-feeding  macrohenthos  near 
abyssal  hydrothermal  vents  at  oceanic  spreading  centers.  Deep-Sea 
Res.  24:  857-863. 

Lutz,  R.  A.  1988.  Dispersal  of  organisms  at  deep-sea  hydrothermal 
vents.  Pp.  23-29  in  Proceedings  of  the  Hydrothermalism,  Biology 
and  Ecology  Symposium,  Pans.  4-7  November  1985.  Oceanol  Ada 
Special  Vol.  No.  8. 

Lutz,  R.  A.,  P.  Bouchet,  D.  Jablonski,  R.  D.  Turner,  and  A.  Waren. 
1986.  Larval  ecology  of  mollusks  at  deep-sea  hydrothermal  vents. 
Am.  Malacol.  Bull  4:  49-54. 

Lutz,  R.  A.,  L.  W.  Fritz,  and  D.  C.  Rhoads.  1985.  Molluscan  growth 
at  deep-sea  hydrothermal  vents.  In  The  Hydrothermal  I'ents  of  the 
Eastern  Pacific':  An  Overview.  M.  L.  Jones,  ed.  Biol.  Soc.  Wash.  Bull. 
6:  199-210. 

Lutz,  R.  A.,  D.  Jablonski,  D.  C.  Rhoads,  and  R.  D.  Turner.  1980.  Larval 
dispersal  of  a  deep-sea  hydrothermal  vent  bivalve  from  the  Galapagos 
Rid.  Alar.  Biol.  57:  127-133. 

Lutz,  R.  A.,  D.  Jablonski,  and  R.  D.  Turner.  1984.  Larval  development 
and  dispersal  at  deep-sea  hydrothermal  vents.  Science  226:  1451- 
1454. 

MacGinitie,  G.  E.  1955.  Distribution  and  ecology  of  the  marine  in- 
vertebrates of  Point  Barrow,  Alaska.  Smilhson.  Misc.  Colled.  128(9): 
1-201. 

Macintosh,  R.  A.  1979.  Egg  capsule  and  young  of  the  gastropod  Ber- 
ingius  bermgii  (MiddendorfT)  (Neptuneidae).  I'eliger  21:  439-441. 

Macintosh,  R.  A.  1986.  Egg  capsule  and  young  of  the  gastropod  Ber- 
nigins  (Neoberingius)  Inelei  (Dall)  (Neptuneidae).  }'eliger  28:  426- 
428. 

Marcus,  E.  B.-R.,  and  E.  Marcus.  1962.  Studies  on  Columbellidae. 
Bui.  Fac  Filos  dene.  Lei  L'niv.  Sao  Paulo  Ser  Zool  24:  335-402. 

Mayer,  L.  A.,  A.  N.  Shor,  J.  Hughes  Clarke,  and  D.  J.  \V.  Piper. 
1988.  Dense  biological  communities  at  3850  m  on  the  Laurentian 
Fan  and  their  relationship  to  the  deposits  of  the  1929  Grand  Banks 
earthquake.  Deep-Sea  Res.  35:  1235-1246. 

McLean,  J.  H.  1988.  New  archaeogastropod  limpets  from  hydrother- 
mal vents.  Superfamily  Lepetodnlacea.  Part  1:  Systematic  descrip- 
tions. Philos.  Trans.  R  Soc.  Land.  B  Biol  Sfi.  319:  1-32. 

McLean,  J.  H.  1989a.  New  archaeogastropod  limpets  from  hydro- 
thermal  vents:  New  family  Peltospindae,  new  superfamily  Peltospi- 
racea.  Zool.  Scr.  18:  49-66. 

Mcl^ean,  J.ll.  1989b.  New  slit  limpets  (Scissurellacea  and  Fissurellacea) 
from  hydrothermal  vents.  Part  1 .  Systematic  descriptions  and  com- 
parisons based  on  shell  and  radular  characters.  Contrib.  Sci.  (Los 
Angel.)  407:  1-29. 

McLean,  J.  H.,  and  G.  Haszprunar.  1987.  Pyropeltidae,  a  new  family 
of  cocculiniform  limpets  from  hydrothermal  vents.  I'eliger 30:  196- 
205. 

Ockelmann,  K.  VV.  1965.  Developmental  types  in  marine  bivalves  and 
their  distribution  along  the  Atlantic  coast  of  Europe.  Pp.  25-35  in 
Proceedings  of  the  First  European  Malacological  Congress.  London. 
1962.  L.  R.  Cox  and  J.  F.  Peake,  eds.  Conchological  Society  of  Great 
Britain  and  Ireland  and  the  Malacological  Society  of  London.  London. 

O'Foighil,  D.  1989.  Planktotrophic  larval  development  is  associated 
with  a  restricted  geographic  range  in  Lasaea,  a  genus  of  brooding. 
hermaphroditic  bivalves.  Alar.  Biol.  103:  349-358. 

Palmer,  A.  R.,  and  R.  R.  Strathmann.  1981.  Scale  ol  dispersal  in  varying 
environments  and  its  implications  for  life  histories  of  marine  inver- 
tebrates. Oecologia  48:  308-318. 


Paull,  C.  K.,  B.  1  lecker,  R.  C'ommeau.  R.  P.  Kreeman-Lynde,  C.  Neumann, 
W.  P.  Corso,  S.  Golubic.  J.  E.  Hook,  E.  Sikes,  and  J.  Curray. 
1984.  Biological  communities  at  the  Florida  Escarpment  resemble 
hydrothermal  vent  taxa.  Science  226:  965-967. 

Pa«lik,  J.  R.,  J.  B.  O'SulIivan,  and  M.  G.  Harasewych.  1988.  The  egg 
capsules,  embryos,  and  larvae  of  Cancellaria  cooperi  (Gastropoda: 
Cancellariidae).  Nautilus  102:  47-53. 

Pechenik,  J.  A.  1986.  The  encapsulation  of  eggs  and  embryos  by  mol- 
luscs: an  overview.  Am.  Malacol.  Bull.  4:  165-172. 

Penchaszadeh,  P.  E.  1982.  Reproductive  aspects  of  Polystira  barretli 
(Guppy.  1866)  (Gastropoda,  Turridae)  from  Golfo  Triste,  Venezuela. 
1'eliger  25:  160-162. 

Perron,  F.  E.  1981.  Larval  biology  of  six  species  of  the  genus  Conns 
(Gastropoda:  Toxoglossa)  in  Hawaii.  USA.  Afar.  Biol.  61:  215-220. 

Perry,  L.  M.,  and  J.  S.  Schwengel.  1955.  Marine  shells  of  the  western 
coast  of  Florida.  Bull  Am  Paleonlol  26:  1-318. 

Petit,  G.,  and  J.  Risbec.  1929.  Sur  la  ponte  de  quelques  Gasteropodes 
Prosobranches.  Bull.  Soc.  Zool  Fr  54:  564-570. 

Pilkington,  M.  C.  1974.  The  eggs  and  hatching  stages  of  some  New 
Zealand  prosobranch  molluscs.  /  R  Soc.  N.Z.  4:  41 1-431. 

Powell,  A.  \V.  B.  1942.  The  New  Zealand  recent  and  fossil  Mollusca 
of  the  family  Turndae.  Bull.  Aiickl  Insl.  Alus.  No.  2:  1-188. 

Radwin,  G.  E.,  and  J.  L.  Chamberlin.  1973.  Patterns  of  larval  devel- 
opment in  stenoglossan  gastropods.  Traits.  San  Diego  Soc.  Nat.  Hist 
17:  107-1  18. 

Rex,  M.  A.,  and  A.  Waren.  1982.  Planktotrophic  development  in  deep- 
sea  prosobranch  snails  from  the  western  North  Atlantic.  Deep-Sea 
Res  29:  171-184. 

Richter.  G.,  and  G.  Thorson.  1975.  Pelagische  prosobranchier-larven 
des  Golfes  von  Neapel.  Ophelia  13:  109-185. 

Robertson,  R.  1971.  Scanning  electron  microscopy  of  planktonic  larval 
marine  gastropod  shells.  Veliger\4:  1-12. 

Robertson.  R.  1976.  Marine  prosobranch  gastropods:  larval  studies  and 
systematics.  Tlialass.  Jugoslav  10:  213-238. 

Rodriguez  Babio,  C.,  and  C.  Thiriot-Quievreux.  1974.  Gasteropodes 
de  la  Region  de  Roscoff.  Etude  particuliere  de  la  protoconque.  Cah. 
Biol  .Mar  15:  531-549. 

Roller,  R.  A.,  and  W.  B.  Stickle.  1988.  Intracapsular  development  of 
Thais  haemastoma  canaliculata  (Gray)  (Prosobranchia;  Muricidae) 
under  laboratory  conditions.  Am.  Malacol.  Bull.  6:  189-197. 

Safriel,  U.  N.,  and  M.  G.  Hadfield.  1988.  Sibling  speciation  by  life- 
history  divergence  in  Dendropoma  (Gastropoda;  Vermetidae).  Biol. 
J  Linn.  Soc.  35:  1-13. 

Schein-Fatton,  E.  1985.  Decouverte  sur  la  ride  du  Pacifique  oriental 
a  13°N  d'un  Pectinidae  (Bivalvia.  Pteromorpha)  d'affinites  paleo- 
zoiques.  C  R  Hehd  Seances  Acad.  Sci.  Ser.  Ill  301:  491-496. 

Scheltema,  A.  H.  1969.  Pelagic  larvae  of  New  England  gastropods.  IV. 
Anachis  translirala  and  Anacllis  avara  (Columbellidae,  Prosobran- 
chia). I'ie  Milieu  Ser.  A  Biol  Mar.  20:  94-104. 

Scheltema,  R.  S.  1978.  On  the  relationship  between  dispersal  of  pelagic 
veliger  larvae  and  the  evolution  of  marine  prosobranch  gastropods. 
Pp.  303-322  in  Marine  Organisms.  B.  Battaglia  and  J.  A.  Beardmore. 
eds.  Plenum,  New  York. 

Scheltema,  R.  S.  1981.  The  significance  of  larval  dispersal  to  the  evo- 
lution ot  benthic  marine  species.  Pp.  130-145  in  Genetics  and  Re- 
production of  Marine  Organisms.  V.  L.  Kasyanov  and  A.  I.  Poduvkin. 
eds.  Proc.  XIV  Pacific  Sci.  Congr.  Khabarovsk.  Aug.  1979,  Section 
Marine  Biology,  Issue  2,  Acad.  Sci.  USSR. 

Scheltema,  R.  S.,  and  I.  P.  Williams.  1983.  Long-distance  dispersal  of 
planktonic  larvae  and  the  biogeography  and  evolution  of  some  Poly- 
nesian and  western  Pacific  mollusks.  Bull.  Mar.  Sci.  33:  545-565. 

Shimek,  R.  L.  1983a.  Biology  of  the  Northeastern  Pacific  Tumdae.  I. 
Opluodermella.  Alalacologia  23:  281-312. 


HVDROTHERMAL  VENT  EGG  CAPSULES 


55 


Shimek,  R.  L.  I983b.     Biology  of  the  Northeastern  Pacific  Turndae.  II. 

Oenopoui  ./  Moll  Stud  49:  146-163. 
Shimek,  R.  L.  I983c.     Biology  of  the  Northeastern  Pacific  Turridae. 

111.  The  habit  and  diet  of  Kurtziella plumbea  (Hinds.  1843).  I'eliger 

26:  10-17. 
Shimek,  R.  I,.  1986.     Biology  of  the  Northeastern  Pacific  Turridae.  IV. 

Demersal  development,  synchronous  settlement  and  other  aspects 

of  the  larval  biology  of  Oenopota  /evidensis  Int  ./  I  men  Reprod 

10:  313-333. 

Shuto,  T.  1974.     Larval  ecology  of  prosobranch  gastropods  and  its  bear- 
ing on  biogeography  and  paleontology.  Lethaia  7:  239-256. 
Smith,  C.  R.,  H.  Kukert,  R.  A.  \\heatcroft,  P.  A.  Jumars.  and  J.  W. 

Drilling.  1989.     Vent  fauna  on  whale  remains.  Nature  341:  27-28. 
Sohl,  N.  F.  1977.     Utility  of  gastropods  in  biostratigraphy.  Pp.  519- 

539  in  Concepts  and  Methods  in  Biostratigraphy.  E.  G.  KaulTman 

and  J.  E.  Hazel,  eds.  Dowden,  Hutchmson  &  Ross.  Stroudsburg, 

Pennsylvania. 
Soliman,  G.  N.  1987.     A  scheme  for  classifying  gastropod  egg  masses 

with  special  reference  to  those  from  the  northwestern  Red  Sea.  / 

Moll  Slttii.  53:  1-12. 
Steele,  D.  H.  1977.     Correlation  between  egg  size  and  developmental 

period.  Am.  \ai.  111:  371-372. 
Strathmann.  M.  F.  1987.     Phylum  Mollusca.  Class  Gastropoda.  Subclass 

Prosobranchia.  Pp.  220-267  in  Reproduction  and  Development  of 

Marine  Invertebrates  of  the  Northern  Pacific  Const  Univ.  of  Wash- 
ington Press,  Seattle  and  London. 
Strathmann,  R.  R.  1977.     Egg  size,  larval  development,  and  juvenile 

size  in  benthic  marine  invertebrates.  Am.  Nat.  Ill:  373-376. 
Strathmann,  R.  R.  1978a.     The  evolution  and  loss  of  feeding  larval  stages 

of  marine  invertebrates.  Evolution  32:  894-906. 
Strathmann,  R.  R.  I978b.     Progressive  vacating  of  adaptive  types  during 

the  Phanerozoic.  Evolution  32:  907-914. 
Stralhmann,  R.  R.  1987.     Larval  feeding.  Pp.  465-550  in  Reproduction 

of  Marine  Invertebrates,  I'oL  IX.  Genera/ Aspects  Seeking  L'nity  in 

Diversity.  Blackwell.  Palo  Alto.  California  and  Boxwood  Press.  Pacific 

Grove.  California. 
Thorson,  G.  1935.     Studies  on  the  egg-capsules  and  development  of 

arctic  marine  prosobranchs.  Medd.  Grant.  100:  1-71. 
Thorson,  G.  1940a.     Studies  on  the  egg  masses  and  larval  development 

of  Gastropoda  from  the  Iranian  Gulf.  Dan  Invest  Iran  2:  159-238. 
Thorson,  G.  1940b.     Notes  on  the  egg  capsules  of  some  North-Atlantic 

Prosobranchs  of  the  genus  Troschelia.  Chrysodomus,  \'olutopsis.  Si- 

phoand  Trophon.  I'idensk.  Medd.  Dan.  Naturhisl.  Foren.  104:  251- 

266. 
Thorson,  G.  1944.     The  zoology  of  east  Greenland:  Marine  Gastropoda 

Prosobranchiata.  Medd.  Grtml  121:  1-181. 


Thorson,  G.  1946.  Reproduction  and  larval  development  of  Danish 
marine  bottom  invertebrates,  with  special  reference  to  the  planktonic 
larvae  in  the  Sound  (Oresund).  Medd  Dan  Eisk  Havunders  Scnc 
Plankton  4:  1-523. 

Thorson,  G.  1950.  Reproduction  and  larval  ecology  of  marine  bottom 
invertebrates.  Bio/  Rev  25:  1-45. 

Todd,  C.  D.,  and  R.  \V.  Doyle.  1981 .  Reproductive  strategies  of  marine 
benthic  invertebrates:  a  settling-timing  hypothesis.  Afar.  Ecol.  Prog 
Ser  4:  75-83. 

Tunnicliffe,  V.  1988.  Biogeography  and  evolution  of  hydrothermal- 
vent  fauna  in  the  eastern  Pacific  Ocean.  Proe.  R  Soc.  Loud.  B  233: 
347-366. 

Tunnicliffe,  V.,  and  S.  K.  Juniper.  1990.  Cosmopolitan  underwater 
fauna.  Nature  344:  300. 

Tunnicliffe,  V.,  and  A.  R.  Fontaine.  1987.  Faunal  composition  and 
organic  surface  encrustations  at  hydrothermal  vents  on  the  southern 
Juan  de  Fuca  Ridge.  ./.  Geophvs  Res  92(B1 1):  1 1.303-1  1,314. 

Tunnicliffe,  \  .,  S.  K.  Juniper,  and  M.  F.  de  Burgh.  1985.  The  hydro- 
thermal  vent  community  on  Axial  Seamount,  Juan  de  Fuca  Ridge. 
In  The  Ilvtlroihennal  I  iwv  ot  the  Eastern  Pacific:  An  Overview. 
M.  L.  Jones,  ed.  Biol.  Soc.  Hash  Bull.  6:  199-210. 

Turner,  R.  D.,  and  R.  A.  l.utz.  1984.  Growth  and  distribution  of  mol- 
lusks  at  deep-sea  vents  and  seeps.  Oceanus  27:  55-62. 

Turner,  R.  D.,  R.  A.  Lutz,  and  D.  Jablonski.  1985.  Modes  of  molluscan 
larval  development  at  deep-sea  hydrothermal  vents.  In  The  Hydro- 
thermal  I  cuts  o/  the  Eastern  I'acilic  An  Oven-ie\\:  M.  L.  Jones,  ed. 
Biol.  Soc  Hash  Hull  6:  167-184. 

Underwood,  A.  J.  1974.  On  models  for  reproductive  strategy  in  marine 
benthic  invertebrates.  Am  Hal  108:  874-878. 

Vance,  R.  R.  1973.  On  reproductive  strategies  in  marine  benthic  in- 
vertebrates. Am.  Nat.  107:  339-352. 

Vance,  R.  R.  1974.     Reply  to  Underwood.  Am.  Nat   108:  879-880. 

Vaught.K.C.  1989.  .1  Classification  of  the  Living  Mollusca.  Melbourne. 
Florida.  American  Malacologists.  189  pp. 

Vestergaard,  K.  1935.  Uber  den  Laich  und  die  Larven  von  Sea/aria 
cominuius  (Lam.),  h'assanus  pygmaeus  (Lam.)  und  Be/a  turricola 
(Mont.).  Zool  An:.  109:  217-222. 

\Varen,  A.,  and  P.  Bouchet.  1986.  Four  new  species  of  Provanna  Dall 
(Prosobranchia.  Centhiacea?)  from  East  Pacific  hydrothermal  sites. 
/ool.  Scr  15:  157-164. 

\Varen.  A.,  and  P.  Bouchet.  1989.  New  gastropods  from  East  Pacific 
hydrothermal  vents.  Zool.  Scr  18:  67-102. 

Webber,  H.  H.  1977.  Gastropoda:  Prosobranchia.  Pp.  1-97  in  Repro- 
duction of  Marine  Invertebrates.  I'ol.  4.  Molluscs:  Gastropods  and 
Cephalopods.  A.  C.  Giese  and  J.  S.  Pearse.  eds.  Academic  Press.  New 
York. 


Reference:  Biol.  Bull-  180:  56-64.  (February.  1'i'H  I 


Expansion  of  the  Sperm  Nucleus  and  Association 

of  the  Maternal  and  Paternal  Genomes  in 

Fertilized  Mulinia  lateralis  Eggs 


FRANK  J.  LONGO1  AND  JOHN  SCARPA2 

*  Department  of  Anatomy.  The  University  of  Iowa.  Iowa  City.  Iowa  52242  and  2  Rutgers  Shellfish 
Research  Laboratory.  Port  Norris,  New  Jersey  08349 


Abstract.  Sperm  nuclear  expansion,  meiotic  maturation 
of  the  maternal  chromatin,  and  events  involving  the  as- 
sociation of  the  male  and  female  pronuclei  leading  to  the 
two-cell  stage  were  observed  in  Mulinia  zygotes  using  the 
fluorochromes  DAPI  and  Hoechst.  The  effects  of  ultra- 
violet irradiation  on  the  fertilizing  sperm  were  also  ex- 
amined. Incorporated  sperm  nuclei  underwent  changes 
in  diameter  that  were  temporally  correlated  with  meiotic 
processes  of  the  maternal  chromatin.  Following  its  entry, 
the  sperm  nucleus  underwent  a  rapid,  initial  enlargement, 
which  was  correlated  with  germinal  vesicle  breakdown. 
Sperm  nuclear  expansion  ceased  during  the  period  in 
which  the  egg  was  engaged  in  polar  body  formation  and 
was  re-initiated  with  formation  and  enlargement  of  the 
female  pronucleus.  The  rates  of  enlargement  of  the  male 
and  female  pronuclei  were  0.59  and  0.65  ^m/min,  re- 
spectively. Following  their  migration  into  apposition  with 
one  another,  the  male  and  female  pronuclei  synchro- 
nously underwent  events  characteristic  of  prophase  as 
separate  structures;  i.e..  chromosome  condensation,  and 
nuclear  envelope  breakdown.  The  two  groups  of  chro- 
mosomes that  formed  became  organized  on  the  metaphase 
plate  in  preparation  of  the  first  cleavage  division;  hence, 
there  was  no  fusion  of  pronuclei.  Ultraviolet  irradiation 
of  fertilizing  sperm  had  no  apparent  affect  on  sperm  nu- 
clear transformations  leading  to  the  development  of  a  male 
pronucleus  or  on  female  pronuclear  development.  How- 
ever, events  subsequent  to  the  apposition  of  the  pronuclei 
were  affected  and  included  asynchrony  of  prophase  and 
the  nondisjunction  of  chromosomes  at  anaphase.  These 
observations  are  discussed  in  relationship  to  events  reg- 


Received  17  September  1990;  accepted  29  November  1990. 


ulating  transformations  of  the  sperm  nucleus  and  exper- 
iments to  generate  gynogenetic  bivalve  embryos. 

Introduction 

For  the  eggs  of  most  animals  insemination  occurs  at 
an  arrested  stage  of  meiosis;  i.e.,  meiotic  prophase  (the 
germinal  vesicle  stage),  metaphase  I,  or  metaphase  II 
(Longo,  1987a).  Representatives  of  these  three  stages  in- 
clude the  eggs  of  annelids,  mollusks,  and  chordates,  re- 
spectively. In  comparison,  the  eggs  of  relatively  few  or- 
ganisms are  fertilized  following  the  completion  of  meiotic 
maturation  (the  pronuclear  stage).  The  most  notable  ex- 
ample of  the  latter  group  are  eggs  of  echinoids.  Although 
processes  of  fertilization  are  fundamentally  the  same  in 
eggs  inseminated  at  different  stages  of  meiotic  maturation, 
there  are  prominent  differences,  particularly  during  the 
transformation  of  the  sperm  nucleus  into  a  male  pronu- 
cleus and  pronuclear  association  (Wilson,  1925;  Longo. 
1985). 

In  eggs  inseminated  at  the  completion  of  meiotic  mat- 
uration, the  female  pronucleus  is  already  present  and 
"waiting"  for  the  entry  and  transformation  of  the  sperm 
nucleus  into  a  pronucleus.  In  contrast,  in  eggs  inseminated 
at  an  arrested  stage  of  meiotic  maturation,  the  sperm  nu- 
cleus, following  its  entry  into  the  egg  cytoplasm,  must 
"wait"  for  the  maternal  chromatin  to  complete  its  meiotic 
maturation.  Observations  carried  out  with  the  gametes  of 
a  variety  of  organisms  (sea  urchin,  surf  clam,  mussel, 
hamster,  rabbit,  and  mouse)  have  shown  that  both  the 
kinetics  of  sperm  nuclear  enlargement  into  a  male  pro- 
nucleus  and  events  attending  pronuclear  association  are 
correlated  with  the  stage  of  meiosis  at  which  the  egg  is 
inseminated  and  the  length  of  time  the  sperm  nucleus 
spends  in  the  egg  cytoplasm  before  pronuclear  association 


56 


FERTILIZATION   EVENTS  IN  MULINIA 


(Wilson,  1925;  Longo,  1985).  For  example,  in  eggs  in- 
seminated at  the  pronuclear  stage,  the  rate  of  sperm  nu- 
clear expansion  is  uniform,  whereas  in  eggs  inseminated 
at  an  arrested  stage  of  meiosis,  the  rate  of  expansion  is 
much  more  complex  and  shows  different  phases  that  are 
correlated  with  stages  of  meiotic  maturation  of  the  ma- 
ternal chromatin(Luttmer  and  Longo,  1987.  1988;  Wright 
and  Longo,  1988;  Longo,  1989). 

In  eggs  inseminated  at  an  arrested  stage  of  meiosis, 
pronuclear  fusion  does  not  occur  as  in  eggs  fertilized  at 
the  pronuclear  stage.  The  paternally  and  maternally  de- 
rived chromatin  do  not  become  associated  with  one  an- 
other until  prophase  of  the  first  cleavage  division  when 
chromosomes  derived  from  the  male  and  female  pronuclei 
intermix  and  become  aligned  on  the  metaphase  plate  of 
the  mitotic  spindle  (Longo,  1985).  Evidence  suggests  that 
differences  in  the  kinetics  of  sperm  nuclear  expansion  and 
pronuclear  association  are  related  to  cell  cycle  events  as- 
sociated with  meiotic  maturation  and  mitosis  of  the  first 
mitotic  division  (Luttmer  and  Longo,  1988;  Wright  and 
Longo,  1988;  Longo,  1989). 

Because  analyses  of  sperm  nuclear  expansion  and  its 
relationship  to  meiotic  events  of  the  maternal  chromatin 
have  been  carried  out  in  relatively  few  organisms  (see 
Longo,  1989),  and  to  further  explore  possible  relationships 
between  these  processes  of  fertilization  and  cell  cycle  phe- 
nomena, we  have  initiated  studies  with  a  variety  of  or- 
ganisms, the  eggs  of  which  are  inseminated  at  an  arrested 
stage  of  meiosis.  Here  we  describe  the  course  of  meiotic 
maturation  of  the  maternal  chromatin,  corresponding 
events  of  sperm  nuclear  enlargement,  and  association  of 
the  male  and  female  pronuclei  in  Mulinia  lateralis  (dwarf 
surf  clam  or  coot  clam)  eggs,  which  are  inseminated  at 
meiotic  prophase.  The  effects  of  ultraviolet  irradiation  on 
sperm  nuclear  transformations  and  events  involving  and 
subsequent  to  male  and  female  pronuclear  association 
are  also  presented. 

Material  and  Methods 

Sexually  mature  individuals  of  Mulinia  lateralis.  col- 
lected from  Massey's  Landing,  Delaware,  were  kept  at 
15°C  in  a  recirculating  seawater  system.  Spawning  was 
induced  by  placing  individual  animals  into  100  ml  beakers 
containing  seawater  at  30°C.  Eggs  from  1  to  3  spawned 
females  were  pooled,  washed  in  fresh  seawater,  insemi- 
nated and  permitted  to  develop  at  20°C.  Unfertilized  eggs 
and  samples  of  fertilized  ova,  taken  at  5  min  intervals  up 
to  1  to  1.5  h  after  the  addition  of  sperm,  were  fixed  in  1% 
formalin  in  seawater.  In  some  experiments,  eggs  and 
sperm  were  incubated  with  10  ^M  Hoechst  33342 
(Hoechst)  in  seawater,  washed  in  fresh  seawater,  and  used 
for  insemination  (Luttmer  and  Longo,  1986). 

To  examine  the  effects  of  ultraviolet  irradiation  on 
sperm  nuclear  transformations  leading  to  pronuclear  de- 


velopment, sperm  were  irradiated  with  ultraviolet  light  as 
previously  described  (Nace  el  a!.,  1970;  Chourrout  and 
Quillet,  1982;ScarpaandBolton,  1988).  Sperm  suspended 
in  a  plastic  Petri  dish  were  exposed  for  20  min,  at  a  dis- 
tance of  20  cm,  to  a  15-watt  tube  generating  ultraviolet 
light  ranging  from  200  nm  to  295  nm,  with  60%  of  the 
ultraviolet  light  concentrated  at  254  nm.  Irradiated  sperm 
were  mixed  with  a  suspension  of  eggs;  samples  were  taken 
and  fixed  as  described  above  for  non-irradiated  specimens. 

Fixed  specimens  were  washed  in  seawater  and  stained 
with  one  of  the  following  DNA  intercalating  fluoro- 
chromes:  1  Mg/mL  4',6-diamidino-2-phenylindole  (DAPI) 
in  seawater,  or  10  ^Af  Hoechst  in  seawater.  Stained  spec- 
imens were  washed  once  in  seawater,  placed  into  a  droplet 
of  glycerol  on  a  glass  slide,  and  covered  with  a  glass  cov- 
erslip.  To  improve  microscopic  observation,  some  spec- 
imens were  compressed.  Specimens  were  observed  with 
a  Nikon  Diaphot  microscope  equipped  with  epifluorosc- 
ence.  Photographs  were  taken  of  specimens  using  40X  or 
100X  objectives  and  Kodak  T-Max  film. 

Because  Mulinia  sperm  nuclei  are  spheroid,  and  their 
transformations  leading  to  male  pronuclei  produced  a 
symmetrical  distribution  of  chromatin  (i.e.,  spheroid), 
changes  in  the  size  of  incorporated  sperm  nuclei  were 
measured  throughout  the  period  of  fertilization.  To  mea- 
sure incorporated  sperm  nuclei  at  different  periods  after 
insemination,  as  well  as  the  developing  spheroid  female 
pronucleus,  stained  specimens  were  placed  into  droplets 
of  glycerol  as  described  above.  A  coverslip,  bearing  a  thin 
layer  of  Vaseline  along  its  edges,  was  lowered  over  the 
droplets  such  that  the  eggs  or  zygotes  were  suspended  be- 
tween the  slide  and  coverslip.  Images  of  the  cross-sectional 
diameters  of  transforming  sperm  nuclei  and  male  and 
female  pronuclei  were  projected  onto  the  screen  of  a  video 
monitor,  checked  for  linearity,  and  traced  onto  plastic 
sheets  with  a  felt  tip  pen.  The  traced  images  were  analyzed 
with  a  Micro-plan  II  Image  Analysis  System  (Laboratory 
Computer  Systems,  Cambridge,  Massachusetts).  Diame- 
ters and  maximum  cross-sectional  areas  of  transforming 
sperm  nuclei  and  male  and  female  pronuclei  were  mea- 
sured (mean  ±  standard  deviation)  at  5-min  intervals  fol- 
lowing insemination  and  temporally  correlated  with  the 
progression  of  meiotic  maturation,  female  pronuclear  de- 
velopment, and  first  mitosis.  Twenty  to  forty  specimens 
were  measured  at  each  time  point. 

Results 

Structure  of  the  unfertilized  egg  and  spermatozoon 

Unfertilized  Mulinia  eggs  measured  46.4  ±  0.4  /*m  in 
diameter.  When  viewed  with  phase  or  Nomarski  optics 
they  were  seen  to  possess  a  large,  meiotic  prophase  nucleus 
(29.7  ±  1.4  ^m  in  diameter),  the  germinal  vesicle,  which 
usually  contained  a  single,  spheroid  nucleolus  ( 10.4  ±  0.8 


58 


F.  J.  LONGO  AND  J.  SCARPA 


Figures  I  and  2.  Nomarski  (Fig.  1)  and  fluorescent  (Fig.  2)  preparations  of  unfertilized  Mulima  eggs 
showing  germinal  vesicles,  nucleoli  (Nu)  and  meiotic  chromosomes.  Figure  I,  X760;  Figure  2,  X960. 

Figures  3  and  4.  Fertilized  Mulima  eggs  depicting  incorporated  sperm  nuclei  (S)  and  meiotic  chromosomes 
which  are  distributed  throughout  the  germinal  vesicle  (5  min  pi).  Figure  3.  •960;  Figure  4.  x  1500. 

Figure  5.  Zygote  ( 10  min  pi)  in  which  the  meiotic  chromosomes  are  condensing  and  the  sperm  nucleus 
(S)  is  dispersing.  XI 800. 

Figures  6-8.  Zygotes  (15  min  pi)  in  which  the  meiotic  chromosomes  have  become  condensed  and 
organized  on  the  same  optical  plane  (Fig.  7).  The  chromosomes  move  as  a  group  to  the  cortex  in  preparation 
for  polar  body  formation  (Fig.  8).  Figure  6  is  a  Nomarski  preparation  in  which  the  meiotic  chromosomes 
and  incorporated  sperm  nucleus  are  difficult  to  discern;  these  structures  are  intensely  stained  in  Hoechst- 
or  DAPI-prepared  specimens.  Figure  6.  X760;  Figures  7  and  8,  X960. 


FERTILIZATION  EVENTS  IN  MULINIA 


59 


fim  in  diameter)  suspended  in  a  nucleoplasm  (Fig.  1). 
Occasionally,  specimens  containing  two  large  nucleoli 
were  observed.  Unfertilized  eggs  prepared  with  DAPI 
(fixed  eggs)  or  Hoechst  (fixed  or  unfixed  eggs)  observed 
with  epi-fluorescence  were  essentially  identical.  Two  fea- 
tures were  apparent  with  both  methods:  ( 1 )  a  low  back- 
ground staining  of  the  cytoplasm,  and  (2)  a  relatively  in- 
tense staining  of  the  maternal  tetrad  chromosomes.  Tet- 
rads were  distributed  throughout  the  interior  of  the 
germinal  vesicle  such  that  chromosome  number  and  in- 
dividual chromosomal  features  (e.g.,  chiasma)  could  be 
ascertained  (Fig.  2).  Examination  of  whole  mounts  and 
compressed  specimens  revealed  that  the  number  of 
meiotic  chromosomes  in  Mulinia  eggs;  i.e..  the  haploid 
number,  was  19  (see  also  Menzel,  1968;  Scarpa  and  Bol- 
ton,  1988;  Wadatva/.,  1990). 

The  structure  of  Mulinia  sperm  as  examined  by  light 
microscopy  was  similar  to  that  of  other  pelecypods  (Fran- 
zen,  1955).  The  sperm  nucleus  was  spheroidal,  1.7  ±0.1 5 
j/m  in  diameter,  and  contained  a  uniform  distribution  of 
DNA  as  determined  in  fluorochrome  stained  preparations. 

As  was  found  for  the  surf  clam,  Spisula  (Luttmer  and 
Longo,  1986),  living  Mulinia  sperm  or  eggs  treated  with 
Hoechst  33342  could  inseminate  and  develop  with  no 
apparent  ill-effects.  In  living  Mulinia  zygotes  in  which 
only  one  of  the  gametes  was  treated  with  Hoechst  dye 
prior  to  insemination,  staining  of  both  the  maternal  and 
paternal  genomes  was  found  consistently  after  fertiliza- 
tion, indicating  that  the  dye  was  not  remaining  confined 
to  the  nucleus  of  one  gamete.  Unlike  the  situation  in  Spi- 
sula (Luttmer  and  Longo,  1986),  we  were  unable  to 
achieve  exclusive  staining  of  only  one  genome  in  Mulinia 
zygotes.  The  following  account  is  based  on  experiments 
employing  fixed  and  unfixed,  stained  specimens. 

Meiotic  maturation  of  the  maternal  chromatin  leading 
to  development  of  the  female  pronucleus 

The  interaction  of  the  sperm  with  the  egg  initiated  the 
resumption  of  meiotic  maturation  and  development  of 
the  female  pronucleus  in  Mulinia.  Resumption  of  meiotic 
maturation  was  heralded  by  the  breakdown  of  the  nuclear 
envelope  of  the  germinal  vesicle  and  the  disappearance 
of  the  nucleolus  (Figs.  3-6).  These  characteristic  features 


of  germinal  vesicle  breakdown  were  readily  apparent  with 
Nomarski  and  phase  contrast  optics  (Fig.  6),  but  changes 
in  the  structure  and  location  of  the  tetrads  were  much 
more  difficult  to  ascertain.  Meiotic  events  of  the  maternal 
chromosomes  and  transformations  of  the  sperm  nucleus 
were  readily  apparent  with  fluorochrome  stained  Mulinia 
preparations  and  epi-fluorescence  microscopy  (Figs.  3,  4, 
5.  7).  Concomitant  with  germinal  vesicle  breakdown  was 
the  condensation  of  the  tetrads  (Figs.  5,  7).  The  tetrads 
formed  a  cluster  within  the  center  of  the  egg;  eventually 
they  were  organized  on  the  metaphase  plate  of  the  first 
meiotic  spindle  (Figs.  7,  8).  The  spindle  and  tetrads  then 
moved  to  one  pole  of  the  egg  where  completion  of  meiosis 
and  polar  body  formation  occurred  (Fig.  9).  In  almost  all 
cases  examined,  more  than  90%  of  the  specimens  were  in 
synchrony  and  had  developed  to  metaphase  I  by  15  min 
postinsemination  (pi). 

Anaphase  I  followed  localization  of  the  meiotic  spindle 
to  the  egg  cortex  and  was  seen  as  the  separation  of  two 
fluorescent  masses  of  chromosomes  (Figs.  9,  10).  With 
the  completion  of  anaphase  I,  the  chromosomes  emitted 
within  the  first  polar  body  formed  a  compact  mass;  those 
within  the  egg  became  reorganized  on  a  metaphase  plate 
in  preparation  for  second  polar  body  formation  (Figs. 
11,  12). 

Anaphase  II  quickly  followed  formation  of  the  first  po- 
lar body  (Fig.  13)  and  appeared  as  the  separation  of  two 
fluorescent  masses  that  were  of  less  intensity  than  the 
chromosomal  masses  that  formed  at  anaphase  I,  reflecting 
the  decrease  in  DNA.  After  anaphase  II,  chromosomes 
within  the  second  polar  body  formed  a  densely  stained 
cluster  (Fig.  14).  The  first  and  the  second  polar  bodies 
became  positioned  side  by  side  and  remained  at  the  pole 
of  the  egg  where  the  meiotic  divisions  took  place. 

By  35  min  pi,  more  than  95%  of  the  specimens  ex- 
amined had  completed  polar  body  formation  and  were 
engaged  in  the  formation  of  a  female  pronucleus  (Figs. 
14-16).  The  maternal  chromosomes  remaining  in  the  egg 
dispersed,  forming  an  irregularly  shaped  nucleus  that 
eventually  expanded  to  become  a  spheroidal  female  pro- 
nucleus.  Measurements  of  the  female  pronucleus  at  dif- 
ferent times  following  its  formation  (35  to  45  min  pi) 
indicated  that  its  rate  of  expansion  was  0.65  ^m/min  (Fig. 
23).  Its  average  maximal  size,  measured  at  45  min  pi. 


Figures  9a,  b.  Zygote  (25  min  pi)  at  two  optical  planes  depicting  anaphase  I  (Fig.  9a)  and  the  incorporated 
sperm  chromatin  (S).  Note  that  the  latter  has  ceased  dispersion  and  is  smaller  than  the  sperm  nucleus 
depicted  in  Figure  7.  X960. 

Figures  10a,b  and  I  la,  b.  Zygotes  (30  min  pi)  at  two  optical  planes  in  which  the  first  polar  body  (P)  has 
formed  and  the  chromosomes  remaining  in  the  zygote  are  preparing  for  the  second  meiotic  division  (Figs. 
lOa,  1  la).  Figures  lOb  and  1  Ib  are  at  the  level  of  the  incorporated  sperm  nucleus  which  has  ceased  its 
enlargement.  <960. 

Figures  12  and  13.  Zygotes  (35  min  pi)  at  metaphase  II  (Fig.  12)  and  anaphase  II  (Fig.  13).  S,  sperm 
nucleus.  X820. 


14" 


01© 


Figures  14a,  b.  Zygote  (35  min  pi)  at  two  optical  planes  depicting  the  maternal  chromosomes  (arrow) 
that  have  just  completed  their  second  meiotic  division  and  are  dispersing  to  form  the  female  pronucleus 
(Fig.  I4a).  Figure  14b  shows  the  sperm  nucleus  (S).  which  is  enlarging  P.  first  and  second  polar  bodies. 
XI 500. 

Figure  15.  Zygote  (40  min  pi)  at  a  slightly  later  stage  of  pronuclear  development  than  the  egg  depicted 
in  Figure  14  in  which  the  male  (M  land  female  (F)  pronuclei  are  expanding.  P,  first  and  second  polar  bodies. 

xl  900. 

60 


FERTILIZATION  EVENTS  IN  MULINIA 


61 


was  9.8  ±  1.4  //m.  By  50  min  pi  the  female  pronucleus 
had  decreased  in  diameter  (9.4  ±  1.1  ^m )  and  was  engaged 
in  prophase  of  the  first  mitotic  (cleavage)  division  (Figs. 

17,23). 

Transformations  of  incorporated  sperm  nuclei  leading 
to  the  development  of  male  pronuc/ei 

Upon  its  incorporation  into  the  egg  cytoplasm,  the 
sperm  nucleus  underwent  an  expansion  from  1.7  ±0.15 
to  3.7  ±  0.28  M"i  in  diameter  (Figs.  3-5,  7,  23).  This 
initial  expansion  occurred  symmetrically  while  the  ma- 
ternal tetrads  were  condensing  and  becoming  aligned  on 
the  metaphase  plate  of  the  first  meiotic  spindle  (Figs.  5, 
7).  From  20  to  35  min  pi,  coincident  with  the  period  in 
which  the  maternal  chromosomes  were  engaged  in  polar 
body  formation,  the  incorporated  sperm  nucleus  did  not 
expand  and,  in  fact,  decreased  slightly  in  size  to  3.5  ±  0.28 
l/m  in  diameter  (Figs.  9-12,  23).  With  the  completion  of 
meiosis  and  the  development  of  the  female  pronucleus 
there  was  a  dramatic  enlargement  in  the  sperm  nucleus 
(rate  =  0.59  yum/min;  Figs.  14-16,  23).  At  the  completion 
of  its  expansion  (45  min  pi),  the  male  pronucleus  mea- 
sured 9.7  ±  1.4  ^m  in  diameter.  Subsequent  changes  in 
the  male  pronucleus  included  its  reduction  in  size  (9.6 
±  0.8  nm)  as  a  part  of  prophase  of  the  first  cleavage  di- 
vision. 

Morphogenesis  of  the  male  and  female  pronuclei 
leading  to  the  first  cleavage  division 

By  45  min  pi,  both  the  male  and  female  pronuclei  had 
reached  their  maximal  sizes  (Fig.  23)  and  individually 
and  synchronously  undergone  prophase  events  leading  to 
the  first  cleavage  division  (Fig.  17).  By  50  min  pi  con- 
densing chromosomes  appeared  in  the  two  pronuclei.  As 
the  chromosomes  condensed,  the  nuclear  envelopes  broke 
down,  forming  two  distinct  groups  of  chromosomes  in 


the  midregion  of  the  zygote,  one  derived  from  the  female 
pronucleus  and  the  other  from  the  male  (Fig.  17).  The 
two  groups  of  chromosomes  moved  together,  intermixed, 
and  became  positioned  on  the  metaphase  plate  of  the  first 
mitotic  spindle  (Fig.  17).  Subsequent  morphogenesis  of 
the  maternally  and  paternally  derived  chromosomes  in- 
volved their  participation  in  the  first  cleavage  division, 
which  was  asymmetric  with  respect  to  cytokinesis  (Fig. 
18).  That  is,  the  metaphase  plate  was  displaced  from  the 
center  of  the  zygote  and,  as  a  consequence,  two  unequally 
sized  blastomeres  formed  upon  cleavage. 

Effects  of  ultraviolet  irradiation  on  male  pronuc/ear 
development  and  morphogenesis 

Effects  of  ultraviolet  irradiation  were  not  apparent  dur- 
ing transformation  of  the  sperm  nucleus  into  a  male  pro- 
nucleus,  nor  was  there  any  apparent  effect  on  meiotic 
maturation  and  development  of  the  female  pronucleus. 
Irradiated  sperm  nuclei  expanded  into  pronuclei  of  a  size 
comparable  to  those  of  control  preparations.  Effects  of 
ultraviolet  irradiation  on  sperm  nuclei  were  not  observed 
until  the  male  pronucleus  was  engaged  in  prophase  events 
of  the  first  cleavage  division.  In  eggs  inseminated  with 
ultraviolet  irradiated  sperm,  mitotic  prophase  events  in 
the  two  pronuclei  were  asynchronous  (Fig.  19).  The  ma- 
ternally and  paternally  derived  chromosomes  eventually 
became  aligned  on  a  metaphase  plate,  but  anaphase  of 
mitosis  was  abnormal  as  evidenced  by  chromosomal 
nondisjunction  (Figs.  20,  21).  The  number  of  chromo- 
somes that  failed  to  move  to  the  spindle  poles  was  not 
constant.  Consequently,  material  of  varying  fluorescent 
intensity  was  seen  between  the  spindle  poles  at  telophase, 
and  between  interconnecting  blastomere  nuclei  at  sub- 
sequent stages  of  development  (Fig.  22). 

Discussion 

The  results  presented  here  demonstrate  nuclear  changes 
that  occur  in  fertilized  Mnlinia  eggs  and  lead  to  the  two- 


Figure  16.  Expanded  male  and  female  pronuclei  that  have  become  associated  with  one  another  in  the 
center  ot  a  zygote  (45  min  pi).  P,  polar  bodies.  X820. 

Figures  17a-e.  Morphogensis  of  the  male  and  female  pronuclei  following  their  apposition.  Initiation  of 
prophase  in  each  pronucleus  is  evident  by  chromosome  condensation  (Fig.  17a,  b).  Two  groups  of  chro- 
mosomes are  produced  (arrows.  Fig.  17c,  d)  which  become  closely  associated  and  positioned  on  the  metaphase 
plate  of  the  first  mitotic  spindle  (Fig.  17e).  P,  polar  bodies.  Figure  17a,  c,  and  d.  X2000;  Figure  17b,  XI 830; 
Figure  17e,  X870. 

Figures  18a-c.  First  cleavage  division  of  Mnlinia  leading  to  unequal  size  blastomers  (Fig.  I8c).  Figures 
18a  and  b  depict  early  and  late  anaphase.  P.  polar  bodies;  B,  developing  blastomere  nuclei.  X870. 

Figure  19.  Asynchronous  pronuclear  morphogenesis  in  an  egg  fertilized  with  an  ultraviolet  irradiated 
sperm.  x750. 

Figure  20.  Nondisjunction  of  mitotic  chromosomes  in  an  egg  inseminated  with  an  ultraviolet  irradiated 
sperm.  K1400. 

Figure  21.  Cleaving  egg  which  was  inseminated  with  an  ultraviolet  irradiated  sperm.  Chromatin  is 
spread  between  the  two  developing  blastomere  nuclei.  >  1400. 

Figure  22.  Cleaved  zygote  that  was  fertilized  by  an  ultraviolet  irradiated  sperm.  DAPI  staining  material 
(i.e.,  DNA)  connects  the  two  blastomere  nuclei  (arrows),  x  1700. 


62 


F.  J.  LONGO  AND  J.  SCARPA 


12 


10  - 


8  • 


a. 
ui 

i 


4- 


2  - 


1  0 


— I — 
20 


— i — 
30 


— i — 
40 


— I — 
50 


60 


TIME       (min.) 

Figure  23.  Expansion  (mean  ±  S.D.)  of  incorporated  sperm  nuclei  (•)  and  female  pronuclei  (A)  of 
Mulinia  zygotes.  The  sperm  nucleus  shows  three  periods  of  transformation:  0  to  15,  15  to  35,  and  35  to  45 
min  pi  corresponding  to  periods  encompassing  germinal  vesicle  breakdown,  polar  body  formation,  and 
female  pronuclear  development,  respectively.  The  decrease  in  size  of  the  male  and  female  pronuclei  from 
45  to  50  min  pi  is  correlated  with  the  onset  of  mitotic  prophase  in  both  pronuclei. 


cell  stage.  Meiosis  of  the  maternal  chromatin,  transfor- 
mations of  the  sperm  nucleus,  and  pronuclear  develop- 
ment and  association  are  readily  amenable  to  analysis  in 
specimens  prepared  with  the  DNA  intercalating  dyes 
DAPI  and  Hoechst.  This  suitability  is  due  to  a  combi- 
nation of  factors,  such  as  low  background  of  the  egg  cy- 
toplasm, and  chromosome  size,  number  and  structure 
(Wadae/fl/.,  1990). 

Meiotic  maturation  of  the  maternal  chromatin  of  Mu- 
linia eggs  is  similar  to  that  previously  described  for  other 
mollusks  (Longo,  1983;  Luttmer  and  Longo,  1988).  In- 
teraction of  the  sperm  with  the  egg  induces  germinal  ves- 
icle breakdown.  The  chromosomes  become  organized  on 
the  metaphase  plate  of  the  first  meiotic  spindle  apparatus 
which  then  moves  to,  and  becomes  positioned  within,  the 
egg's  cortex.  The  mechanism  by  which  this  movement 
takes  place  has  not  been  established,  although  investiga- 
tions demonstrating  that  cytochalasin  B  inhibits  the  cor- 
tical localization  of  the  meiotic  spindle  suggests  that  it 
may  be  an  actin-mediated  process  (Longo,  1987b). 

Anaphase  I  and  II,  as  well  as  the  formation  of  the  first 
and  second  polar  bodies,  followed  in  quick  succession,  as 
occurs  in  the  surf  clam  Spisula  (see  Longo,  1983).  For- 


mation of  the  female  pronucleus  was  evident  subsequent 
to  the  formation  of  the  second  polar  body  by  the  formation 
of  an  expanding  mass  of  material  staining  with  either 
DAPI  or  Hoechst.  The  rate  of  expansion  of  the  forming 
female  pronucleus  was  comparable  to  that  of  the  male 
pronucleus,  suggesting  that  the  two  chromatin  masses  may 
be  regulated  by  similar  mechanisms.  A  corresponding  re- 
lationship has  also  been  demonstrated  in  polygynic  and 
polyspermic  Spisula  zygotes  (Luttmer  and  Longo,  1988). 
The  kinetics  of  sperm  nuclear  expansion  in  fertilized 
Mulinia  eggs  is  in  agreement  with  previous  studies  dem- 
onstrating that  sperm  nuclear  transformations  share  a 
temporal  relationship  with  changes  of  the  maternal 
chromatin  (Das  and  Barker,  1976;  Da-Yuan  and  Longo, 
1983;Yamashita,  1985;  Luttmer  and  Longo,  1987,  1988; 
Wright  and  Longo,  1988;  Longo,  1989).  Measurements 
of  sperm  nuclear  expansion  in  Mulinia  zygotes  indicates 
that  this  process  takes  place  in  three  distinct  phases  tem- 
porally correlated  with  meiotic  maturation  of  the  ma- 
ternal chromatin.  In  previous  studies,  as  well  as  in  the 
one  reported  here,  the  incorporated  sperm  nucleus  un- 
dergoes a  period  of  rapid  expansion  followed  by  one  of 
no  enlargement  or  condensation.  This  is  succeeded  by  a 


FERTILIZATION  EVENTS  IN  MVLINIA 


63 


dramatic  expansion  of  the  sperm  nucleus  leading  to  a 
male  pronucleus  similar  in  size  to  that  of  the  female. 
The  three  phases  of  sperm  nuclear  enlargement  in  Mu- 
linia  correlate  with  germinal  vesicle  breakdown,  polar 
body  formation,  and  female  pronuclear  development, 
respectively.  The  kinetics  of  sperm  nuclear  expansion  is 
similar  to  that  described  in  the  surf  clam,  Spisula  so/i- 
disima,  where  four  phases  were  observed  based  on  closer 
sampling  times  than  those  taken  during  the  course  of 
the  present  study  (Luttmer  and  Longo,  1988).  In  Spisula, 
the  sperm  nucleus,  upon  incorporation,  underwent  little 
change  in  size  until  germinal  vesicle  breakdown.  Addi- 
tionally, during  the  period  of  polar  body  formation,  the 
expanded  sperm  nucleus  of  Spisula  underwent  a  signif- 
icant reduction  in  size;  i.e.,  it  condensed.  A  reduction 
(one  time  point)  in  size  of  the  expanded  sperm  nucleus 
of  Mulinia  zygotes  was  observed  during  polar  body  for- 
mation. We  suspect  that  with  closer  sampling  times,  this 
reduction,  as  well  as  the  status  of  the  incorporated  sperm 
nucleus  prior  to  germinal  vesicle  breakdown,  would  be- 
come apparent  in  Mulinia  zygotes. 

Expansion  of  the  sperm  nucleus  following  germinal 
vesicle  breakdown  is  consistent  with  other  studies  dem- 
onstrating that  mixing  of  germinal  vesicle  substances  with 
the  cytoplasm  precedes  sperm  nuclear  changes  (Masui  and 
Clarke,  1979;  Longo,  1981;  Schuetz  and  Longo,  1981; 
Hirait'/d/..  1981;  Yamada  and  Hirai,  1984).  This  change 
in  the  sperm  nucleus  may  be  a  manifestation  of  sperm 
basic  protein  replacement  by  histones  present  in  the  oocyte 
cytoplasm.  Histone  changes  that  occur  with  the  early  onset 
of  sperm  chromatin  dispersion  have  been  demonstrated 
(Poccia  el  al.,  1978,  1981;  see  Poccia,  1986).  Because 
agents  affecting  meiotic  maturation  of  the  maternal  chro- 
matin also  affect  the  kinetics  of  sperm  nuclear  expansion 
(Luttmer  and  Longo,  1988;  Wright  and  Longo,  1988), 
factors  regulating  the  status  of  the  maternal  chromatin 
during  polar  body  formation  probably  act  on  the  trans- 
formed sperm  nucleus  such  that  it  ceases  expansion  and 
in  some  instances  condenses;  e.g ,  surf  clam,  hamster,  and 
starfish  (Luttmer  and  Longo,  1988;  Wright  and  Longo, 
1988;  Longo,  1989). 

The  second  expansion  of  the  sperm  nucleus,  which  is 
correlated  with  enlargement  of  the  maternal  chromatin 
and  female  pronuclear  formation  (Zirkin  el  al.,  1989),  is 
set  into  motion  as  a  result  of  cell  cycle  changes  within  the 
fertilized  egg  that  affect  both  the  maternally  and  paternally 
derived  chromation  (Longo,  1989).  In  the  case  of  Mulinia, 
as  well  as  other  species  that  have  been  studied  to  date, 
both  chromatin  masses  undergo  dramatic  rates  of  expan- 
sion to  form  enlarged  pronuclei  (Luttmer  and  Longo, 
1988;  Wright  and  Longo,  1 988).  Unlike  the  situation  seen 
in  mammalian  zygotes  (Wright  and  Longo,  1988),  ex- 
pansion of  the  maternally  and  paternally  derived  chro- 


matin resulted  in  pronuclei  of  nearly  equal  size  (see  also 
Luttmer  and  Longo,  1989). 

Results  presented  here  demonstrate  that  pronuclear  fu- 
sion in  Mulinia  does  not  occur  as  in  sea  urchins  (fertilized 
at  the  completion  of  meiotic  maturation)  or  as  in  other 
cellular  systems  (Longo  and  Anderson,  1968).  Rather, 
both  the  male  and  female  pronuclei,  as  separate  bodies, 
synchronously  undergo  prophase  events  in  preparation 
for  first  mitosis.  The  chromosomes  from  each  pronucleus, 
which  replicated  during  the  period  following  polar  body 
formation,  become  aligned  on  the  metaphase  plate  of  the 
mitotic  spindle  and  separate  at  anaphase  into  two  masses 
consisting  of  both  maternally  and  paternally  derived 
chromosomes.  Hence,  maternal  and  paternally  derived 
chromosomes  do  not  become  enclosed  within  the  same 
nucleus  until  formation  of  the  two-cell  stage. 

The  effects  of  ultraviolet  irradiation  on  sperm  trans- 
formations that  lead  to  the  development  of  male  pronuclei 
in  Mulinia  are  consistent  with  what  has  been  shown  in 
other  systems  (Onozato  and  Yamaha,  1983;  Arai  el  al., 
1 984).  Ultraviolet  irradiation  disrupts  the  DNA  helix  and 
thus  interferes  with  the  proper  duplication  of  chromo- 
somes prior  to  first  cleavage  (Strickberger,  1976).  We  an- 
ticipated that  the  effects  of  ultraviolet  irradiation  might 
be  manifested  at  two  periods  during  fertilization:  ( 1 )  dur- 
ing transformation  of  the  sperm  nucleus  into  a  male  pro- 
nucleus,  indicative  of  gross  DNA  disruption;  and  (2)  sub- 
sequent to  DNA  replication,  during  the  period  in  which 
the  paternally  derived  chromosomes  were  engaged  in  mi- 
tosis. Alterations  were  not  apparent  during  any  of  the 
stages  leading  to  a  male  pronucleus,  possibly  due  to  an 
insensitivity  of  the  method  of  analysis,  or  more  likely  to 
an  inability  to  achieve  concomitant  high  levels  of  irradia- 
tion and  fertilization.  (Higher  doses  of  ultraviolet  irradia- 
tion were  tested  but  resulted  in  an  inhibition  of  fertiliza- 
tion.) Radiation  effects  were  seen  only  after  pronuclear 
association — i.e..  during  prophase  and  anaphase  of  the 
first  cleavage  division — and  involved  variable  numbers  of 
chromosomes.  The  manner  in  which  the  male  and  female 
pronuclei  become  associated  in  Mulinia  and  other  mol- 
luscan  eggs  (see  Longo,  1983),  as  well  as  parameters  af- 
fecting both  the  quantity  and  quality  of  ultraviolet  irra- 
diation, call  into  question  the  effectiveness  of  using  ultra- 
violet irradiation  to  form  gynogenetic  molluscan  embryos. 
Induction  of  gynogenesis  with  variable  results  has  been 
achieved  by  a  variety  of  techniques,  including  irradiation 
of  sperm  with  ultraviolet  light  (Chourrout,  1980;  Streis- 
inger  el  al.,  1981;  Onozato  and  Yamaha,  1983;  Lou  and 
Purdom,  1984;  Onozato.  1984;  Suzuki  el  al.,  1985).  Vari- 
ability in  cases  employing  ultraviolet  irradiation  (Chour- 
rout, 1980;  Onozato  and  Yamaha,  1983;  Arai  el  al..  1984) 
appeared  to  be  due  to  difficulties  in  controlling  parameters 
associated  with  the  exposure  of  sperm  to  ultraviolet  rays 


64 


F.  J.   LONGO  AND  J.  SCARPA 


and  an  inability  to  uniformly  and  effectively  destroy  all 
of  the  paternally  derived  DNA. 

Acknowledgments 

The  assistance  of  Tena  Perry  and  Lori  Mathews  is 
greatfully  appreciated.  Portions  of  the  study  presented  here 
were  supported  by  funds  from  the  NIH.  Support  for  John 
Scarpa  was  provided  by  the  New  Jersey  Agricultural  Ex- 
periment Station  grant  to  Standish  K.  Allen,  Jr. 

Literature  Cited 

Aral,  K.,  F.  Naito,  H.  Sasaki,  and  K.  Fujino.  1984.  Gynogenesis  with 
ultraviolet  ray  irradiated  sperm  in  the  Pacific  abalone.  Bull  Jpn.  Sue. 
Sci.  Fish.  50:2019-2023. 

Das,  N.  K.,  and  C.  Barker.  1976.  Mitotic  chromosome  condensation 
in  the  sperm  nucleus  during  postfertilization  maturation  division  in 
t'w/i/.veggs.  J.  Cell  BioL  68:  155-159. 

Da-Yuan,  C.,  and  F.  J.  Longo.  1983.  Sperm  nuclear  dispersion  coor- 
dinate with  meiotic  maturation  in  fertilized  Spisti/a  solidixsima  eggs. 
Dev.  Bml  99:  217-244. 

Chourrout,  D.  1980.  Thermal  induction  of  diploid  gynogenesis  and 
tnploidy  in  the  eggs  of  the  rainbow  trout  (Salmo  gairdneri).  Reprod. 
Nutr.  Dev.  20:  727-733. 

Chourrout,  D.,  and  F..  Quillet.  1982.  Induced  gynogenesis  in  the  rainbow 
trout:  sex  and  survival  of  progenies  production  of  all-triploid  popu- 
lations. Them.  Appl.  Genel.  63:  201-205. 

Franzen,  A.  1955.  Comparative  morphological  investigations  into  the 
spermiogenesis  among  Mollusca.  Zool.  Buin.it;.  30:  399-456. 

Hirai,  S.,  Y.  Nagahama,  and  M.  Kanatani.  1981.  Cytoplasmic  maturity 
revealed  by  the  structural  changes  in  incorporated  spermatozoan 
during  the  course  of  starfish  oocyte  maturation.  Dev.  Growth  Differ. 
23:  465-478. 

Longo,  F.  J.  1981.  Regulation  of  pronuclear  development.  Pp.  529- 
557  in  Bioregulalon  of  Reproduction,  G.  Jagiello  and  C.  Vogel.  eds. 
Academic  Press,  New  York. 

Longo,  F.  J.  1983.  Meiotic  maturation  and  fertilization.  Pp.  49-89  in 
The  Mollusca,  Vol.  3,  N.  H.  Verdonk  and  J.  A.  M.  van  den  Biggelaar. 
eds.  Academic  Press.  New  York. 

Longo,  F.  J.  1985.  Pronuclear  events.  Pp.  25 1-298  in  Biology  of  Fer- 
tilization. Vol.  3,  C.  B.  Metz  and  A.  Monroy,  eds.  Academic  Press. 
NY. 

Longo,  F.  J.  1987a.     Fertilization.  Chapman  and  Hall.  New  York. 

Longo,  F.  J.  1987b.  Egg  cortical  architecture.  Pp.  108-138  in  The  Cell 
Biology  of  Fertilization,  G.  Schatten  and  H.  Schatten,  eds.  Academic 
Press.  Inc. 

Longo,  F.  J.  1989.  Dynamics  of  sperm  nuclear  transformations  at  fer- 
tilization. Pp.  297-307  in  fertili:ation  in  Mammals.  B.  D.  Bavister, 
J.  Cummins  and  E.  R.  S.  Roldan.  eds.  Serono  Symposia.  Norwell, 
MA. 

Longo,  F.  J.,  and  E.  Anderson.  1968.  The  fine  structure  of  pronuclear 
development  and  fusion  in  the  sea  urchin  Arhacia  puneiulata.  J.  Cell 
Bio/.  39:  335-368. 

Lou,  Y.  D.,  and  C.  E.  Purdom.  1984.  Diploid  gynogenesis  induced  by 
hydrostatic  pressure  in  the  rainbow  trout,  Salmo  gairdneri.  J.  Fish 
Biol.  24:  665-670. 

Luttmer,  S.  J.  and  F.  J.  Longo.  1986.  Examination  of  living  and  fixed 
gametes  and  early  embryos  stained  with  supervital  Quorochromes 


(Hoechst  33342  and  3,3'-dihexyloxacarboxyanine  iodide).  Gamete 
Res  15:267-283. 

I  mum  i,  S.  J.,  and  F.  J.  Longo.  1987.  Rates  of  male  pronuclear  en- 
largement in  sea  urchin  zygotes.  J.  Exp  Zoul  243:  289-298. 

Luttmer,  S.  J.,  and  F.  J.  Longo.  1988.  Sperm  nuclear  transformations 
consist  of  enlargement  and  condensation  coordinate  with  stages  of 
meiotic  maturation  in  fertilized  Spisu/a  solidissima  oocytes.  Dev. 
BioL  128:  86-96. 

Masui,  Y.,  and  H.  Clarke.  1979.  Oocyte  maturation.  Inl  Rev  Cvtol. 
57:  185-282. 

Menzel,  R.  W.  1968.  Chromosome  number  in  nine  families  of  marine 
pelecypod  mollusks.  Nautilus  82:  45-58. 

Nace,  G.  W.,  C.  M.  Richards,  and  J.  H.  Asher  Jr.  1970.  Parthenogenesis 
and  genetic  variability.  I.  Linkage  and  inbreeding  estimations  in  the 
frog.  Rana  pipiens  Genetics  66:  349-368. 

Onozato,  II.  1984.  Diploidization  of  gynogenetically  activated  salmonid 
eggs  using  hydrostatic  pressure.  Aquaculture  43:  91-97. 

Onozato,  H.,  and  E.  Yamaha.  1983.  Induction  of  gynogenesis  with 
ultraviolet  rays  in  four  species  of  salmoniformes.  Bull.  Jpn.  Soc.  Sci. 
Fish.  49:  693-699. 

Poccia,  D.  1986.  Remodeling  of  nucleoproteins  during  gametogenesis. 
fertilization  and  early  development.  Int.  Rev  Cytol.  105:  1-65. 

Poccia,  D.,  G.  Krystal,  D.  Nishioka,  and  J.  Salik.  1978.  Controls  of 
sperm  chromatin  structure  by  egg  cytoplasm  in  the  sea  urchin.  1CN- 
UCLA  Symp.  Mol.  Cell.  Biol.  12:  197-206. 

Poccia,  D.,  J.  Salik,  and  G.  Krystal.  1981.  Transitions  in  histone  vari- 
ants of  the  male  pronucleus  following  fertilization  and  evidence  lor 
a  maternal  stage  of  cleavage-stage  histones  in  sea  urchin  eggs.  Dev. 
Biol.  82:  287-296. 

Scarpa,  J.,  and  E.  T.  Bolton.  1988.  Experimental  production  of  gyn- 
ogenetic  and  parthenogenetic  Mulinia  lateralis  (Say).  /  Shellfish  Res. 
7:  132. 

Sehuetz,  A.,  and  F.  J.  Longo.  1981.  Hormone-cytoplasmic  interaction 
controlling  sperm  nuclear  decondensation  and  male  pronuclear  de- 
velopment in  starfish  oocytes.  J.  Exp.  Zoo/.  215:  107-1 1 1. 

Streisinger,  G.,  C.  Walker,  N.  Dower,  C.  Dnauber,  and  F.  Singer. 
1981.  Production  of  clones  of  homozygous  diploid  zebra  fish 
Brachydanio  reno.  Nature  291:  293-296. 

Strickberger,  M.  W.  1976.     Genetics.  Macmillan  Pub.  Co..  New  York. 

Suzuki,  R..  I .  Oshiro,  and  T.  Nakanishi.  1985.  Survival,  growth  and 
fertility  of  gynogenetic  diploids  induced  in  the  cypnmd  loach,  Afis- 
giirnus  anguillicaudatus.  Aquaculture  48:  45-55. 

\\ada,  K.  T.,  J.  Scarpa,  and  S.  K.  Allen  Jr.  1990.  Karyotype  of  the 
dwarf  surf  clam  Mulinia  lateralis  (Mactndne.  Bivalvia).  /  Shellfish 
Res  9:  in  press. 

Wilson,  E.  B.  1925.  The  Cell  in  Development  and  Heredity.  McMillian, 
New  York. 

Wright,  S.  J.,  and  F.  J.  Longo.  1988.  Sperm  nuclear  enlargement  in 
fertilized  hamster  eggs  is  related  to  meiotic  maturation  of  the  maternal 
chromatin./  Exp.  Zoo/  247:  155-165. 

Yamada,  II.,  and  S.  Hirai.  1984.  Role  of  contents  of  the  germinal  vesicle 
in  male  pronuclear  development  and  cleavage  of  starfish  oocytes. 
Dev.  Growth  Difier  26:  479-487. 

Yamashita,  M.  1985.  Electron  microscopic  analysis  of  the  sperm  nu- 
clear changes  in  meiosis  inhibited  eggs  of  the  brittle  star,  Amphipholis 
kochii.  J  Exp.  Zool.  235:  105-1 17. 

Zirkin,  B.,  S.  D.  Perrault,  and  S.  J.  Naish.  1989.  Formation  and  func- 
tion of  the  male  pronucleus  during  mammalian  fertilization.  Pp.  91- 
1  14  in  The  Molecular  Biology  of  Fertilization,  H.  Schatten  and  G. 
Schatten.  eds.  Academic  Press.  New  York. 


Reference:  Btol.  Bull.  180:  65-71.  (February. 


Putative  Molt-Inhibiting  Hormone  in  Larvae 

of  the  Shore  Crab  Carcinus  maenas  L.: 

An  Immunocytochemical  Approach 

S.  G.  WEBSTER'  AND  H.  DIRCKSEN* 

School  of  Biological  Sciences,  University  College  of  North  Wales,  Bangor.  Gwynedd  LL57  2UW,  UK, 

an d  *Institut  fiir  Zoophysiologie.  Universitdt  Bonn,  Endenicher  Allee  11-13. 
D-5300  Bonn  1.  Germany. 


Abstract.  Immunocytochemical  investigations  of  the 
eyestalk  of  Carcinus  maenas  zoeal  larval  stages,  using  an 
antiserum  directed  against  putative  Carcinus  molt-inhib- 
iting  hormone  (M1H),  revealed  immunopositive  neuronal 
structures.  These  structures  included  perikarya  associated 
with  the  medulla  terminalis  X-organ,  parts  of  the  sinus 
gland  tract,  and  the  neurohemal  organ — the  sinus  gland. 
Apart  from  an  increase  in  volume  of  the  sinus  gland  be- 
tween zoeal  stage  I  and  II,  no  striking  changes  in  the  to- 
pography or  morphology  of  the  MIH  neurosecretory  sys- 
tem were  observed.  Immunopositive  structures  were 
found  in  similar  locations  to  those  seen  in  adult  crabs. 
Our  results  suggest  that  the  control  of  molting  by  MIH 
in  crustacean  larvae  may  be  similar  to  the  currently  ac- 
cepted model  of  molt  control  in  adult  decapod  crusta- 
ceans. 

Introduction 

A  current  model  of  molt  control  in  decapod  crustaceans 
involves  regulation  of  ecdysteroid  synthesis  by  a  molt- 
inhibiting  hormone  (MIH),  released  by  neurosecretory 
neurons  in  the  eyestalk.  Much  evidence  has  now  accu- 
mulated suggesting  that  increased  synthesis  and  liters  of 
circulating  ecdysteroids  necessary  for  induction  of  premolt 
are  directly  repressed  by  this  neuropeptide,  thus  inhibiting 
proecdysis  and  molting.  Nevertheless,  alternative  hy- 
potheses have  implicated  processes  such  as  metabolism 
and  excretion  of  ecdysteroids  in  molt  regulation  (see 
Skinner,  1985;  Webster  and  Keller,  1988;  Watson  el  ai. 


Received  23  May  1990;  accepted  6  November  1990. 
1  To  whom  correspondence  should  be  sent. 


1989  for  recent  reviews).  Despite  recent  advances  in  our 
knowledge  concerning  mechanisms  of  molt  control  in 
adult  decapod  crustaceans,  little  is  known  about  the  reg- 
ulation of  molting  in  larval  crustaceans.  This  deficiency 
has  been  reiterated  in  a  recent  review  by  Christiansen 
(1988). 

Evidence  for  molt  regulation  by  MIH  in  crustacean 
larvae  has,  until  recently,  been  obtained  by  eyestalk  abla- 
tion experiments  (for  references  see  Charmantier  et  ai, 
1988;  Christiansen  1988),  which  have  given  equivocal  re- 
sults, suggesting  that  in  some  instances,  the  larval  molt  is 
not  regulated  by  MIH  until  shortly  before  metamorphosis. 
However,  with  regard  to  morphological  correlates  of  neu- 
rosecretory structures  in  larval  eyestalks,  several  reports 
(Orlamunder,  1942;  Pyle,  1943;  Hubschman.  1953;  Dahl, 
1957;  Matsumoto,  1958;  Little,  1969;  Zielhorst  and  Van 
Herp,  1976;  Bellon-Humbert  et  a/.,  1978;  Gorgels-Kallen 
and  Meij,  1985)  detail  the  ontogeny  of  larval  neurosecre- 
tory systems  in  a  wide  variety  of  crustaceans.  With  the 
exception  of  studies  by  Gorgels-Kallen  and  Meij  (1985), 
Beltz  and  Kravitz  (1987),  and  Beltz  et  a/.,  (1990),  there 
are  no  other  studies  in  which  neurosecretory  systems  con- 
taining immunocytochemically  defined  neuropeptides 
have  been  described  in  crustacean  larvae. 

Recently,  we  have  characterized  a  neuropeptide  from 
the  sinus  gland  of  Carcinus  maenas,  which,  by  virtue  of 
its  ability  to  repress  ecdysteroidogenesis  by  Y-organs  cul- 
tured in  vitro,  could  be  described  as  a  putative  MIH 
(Webster,  1986;  Webster  and  Keller,  1986).  It  should  be 
stressed  that  the  precise  significance  and  function  of  this 
neuropeptide  as  a  molt-inhibitor  in  vivo  has  not  yet  been 
elucidated,  and  until  suitable  in  vivo  bioassays  are  devel- 
oped, the  status  of  MIH  must  remain  "putative."  Re- 


65 


.•ykiAvis  b 

^' 


C    ' 

•   -  *     _  • . « 


^    !pV*fr      «'• 
<v\J     "    '  (frA 

.  ^ 

:>         ->•/  .     v.s'v"'   $?.  ^  ~> '^V^*,. 

•  ./'^y.  /<;  ••:•  -r  r       A  >^ 

•     '    •  •      •  '  •/'''•' I-  '    '  ^^'-CA  >• 

'  '^:  *  -^'  i'&*Wt?          /\  '*.T,«4 

i--V  W--  ;  '<&      ]  ^       '"       >      ^ 

i  ^   !     .  d          — • 

3 


V 


;  Mi . 
.  •     $& 


* 


*•?    :\ 


. 


I 

U::^S  ^.A|  HI 

,  ••-    -,^       ,>       •  ll::-l 


, 
K 


•••N      .x'-        1 

i   i 

-  K®  $  -,; 


;.•  '.    '. 

"'.•'•  •  ;;ic^ 


\,    .    ;.     / 


f 


.* 


'#  -•'£  '    <-^Y\^^ 

•  '•  ;    i — i        N  ^Uw^  v. 

I 


?"• 


'j;SsrS.:/-o'ci 

'^'.«^feg^ 

>     r  'v     -it  ,4«X   0^,1-"' 

>-     x     .     .^^-'^'. 

~v  •  •    J  nfc/f^v^ 

x^^w* 


Figure  1.  Characteristic  structures  of  MIH-immunoreactive  (IR)  neurons  in  prezoeal  (a,  c),  stage  I  zoea 
(h.  d-f.  left  eye),  and  stage  II  zoea  (g-l)  eyes  of  Carcinus  maenas  larvae.  Phase  contrast  micrographs  of 
immunostained  semithin  ( 1  nm)  transverse  sections.  (Orientation  of  dorsal  parts  of  larvae  to  the  tops  of 
micrographs.) 


66 


MOLT-INHIBITING  HORMONE  IN  CRAB  LARVAE 


67 


cently,  we  demonstrated  that  the  neurosecretory  system 
produced  putative  MIH  in  the  eyestalk  ganglia  of  several 
adult  brachyuran  crustaceans  (Dircksen  el  at..  1988).  Be- 
cause these  studies  provide  compelling  evidence  to  suggest 
that  MIH  is  a  secretable  neuropeptide,  and  in  view  of  our 
earlier  observations  on  the  nature  and  mode  of  action  of 
this  neuropeptide  on  ecdysteroid  synthesis  in  Carcinus 
(Webster  and  Keller.  1986;  Lachaise  el  al.  1989),  it 
seemed  opportune  to  examine  the  larval  eyestalk  neu- 
rosecretory system  immunocytochemically,  using  anti- 
bodies raised  against  Carcinus  MIH.  Evidence  presented 
here  suggests  that  a  functional  MIH-like  neurosecretory 
system  exists  in  all  larval  stages  of  Carcinus. 

Materials  and  Methods 

Laboratory  rearing  of  larvae 

Ovigerous  Carcinus  maenas  L.  females  were  collected 
from  the  Menai  Strait,  North  Wales,  between  May  and 
July,  and  maintained  in  the  laboratory  until  larvae  were 
released.  Only  positively  phototropic,  rapidly  swimming 
larvae  were  collected.  Rearing  techniques  were  initially 
based  upon  those  of  Rice  and  Ingle  (1975),  but  were  found 
to  be  inadequate.  Successful  rearing  to  first  crab  with  a 
high  survival  was  achieved  using  a  mixed  diet  of  (A)  phy- 
toplankton  (Tetraselmis  clniii),  (B)  rotifers  (Brachionus 
plicatilis),  (C)  barnacle  nauplii  (Ehninius  modeslus),  and 
(D)  brine  shrimp  nauplii  (Anemia  salina).  During  each 
larval  stage,  prey  ratios  were  supplied  as  follows:  Zoea  I 


III  (C):l,  (D):l.  Zoea  IV,  Megalopa  and  First  crab  (D):l. 
With  the  exception  of  phytoplankton  (culture  density  ca. 
106  cells  mr  ':  1  part  =  1  5  ml),  the  total  prey  concentration 
was  around  25-50  items  per  ml.  Larvae  were  reared  in 
50-ml  plastic  containers  in  constantly  aerated,  filtered 
seawater  (33%o)  under  ambient  temperature  (15-18°C) 
and  photoperiod  (L  15-18  h:  D9-6  h).  Maximum  density 
of  larvae  was  1  per  5  ml.  Water  and  food  were  changed 
every  two  days,  at  which  time  instars  were  staged  accord- 


ing to  Rice  and  Ingle  (1975).  Under  these  maintenance 
conditions,  survival  was  good  (80%),  and  instar  durations 
were  approximately:  Z  I:  7,  Z  II:  5,  Z  III:  6,  Z  IV:  7,  M: 
8,  days.  Samples  of  larvae  were  taken  at  the  middle  of 
each  instar,  which  was  considered  to  be  during  intermoult. 

Tissue  processing  and  immunocytochemistry 

Fixations  were  carried  out  in  a  mixture  of  2%  parafor- 
maldehyde,  2%  glutaraldehyde.  and  0. 1%  saturated  picric 
acid  in  0. 1  M  sodium  cacodylate  buffer,  pH  7.4,  supple- 
mented with  0.5  M  sucrose  and  5  mM  CaCl2  for  2-4  h 
at  4°C  according  to  Dircksen  et  al.  (1987).  Tissues  were 
washed  extensively  in  the  same  buffer,  dehydrated,  and 
embedded  in  low  viscosity  resin  (Spurr,  1969).  Semithin 
frontal  cross-sections  ( 1  ^m)  through  the  whole  animal 
were  cut  on  a  LKB  Ultrotome  III  or  a  Reichert  Ultracut 
E,  and  processed  for  immunocytochemistry  using  a  rabbit 
antiserum  (code  R1TB)  directed  against  HPLC-purified 
MIH  of  Carcinus  (Dircksen  el  al.,  1988),  diluted  1:4000 
in  0.0 1  M  phosphate  buffered  saline  (PBS)  and  PAP  stain- 
ing techniques  (Dircksen  et  al.,  1987).  Micrographs  were 
taken  with  a  Zeiss  Axioskop  using  phase  contrast  optics 
and  documented  on  Agfapan  25  film. 

Results 

Despite  several  attempts  to  improve  the  penetration  of 
fixative  into  the  eyestalks  (for  example,  by  piercing  the 
exoskeleton  behind  the  eyestalks,  using  other  fixatives  or 
fixation  times),  adequate  fixation  of  megalopae  and  first 
crab  stages  was  impossible.  Thus,  by  necessity,  this  study 
is  restricted  to  the  zoeal  stages  of  Carcinus,  and  in  later 
zoeal  stages  problems  with  fixation  and  tissue  shrinkage 
were  encountered.  A  sometimes  confusing  feature  of  the 
zoeal  eyestalk  was  the  presence  of  a  pigmented  perineural 
sheath  (Fig.  2c,  2f),  which  could  have  been  identified  as 
an  immunopositive  structure.  This  problem  was  resolved 
by  using  normal  bright  field  optics,  under  which  immu- 
nopositive material  appears  brownish,  or  by  higher  mag- 


fa)  MIH-IR  axon  profiles  within  the  sinus  gland  (center  of  rectangle)  of  a  prezoea.  Note  ommatidial  pnmordia. 
brain  (*)  and  yolk  droplets  (arrowhead),  (b)  MIH-IR  axon  profiles  within  the  sinus  gland  (rectangle)  of  a 
stage  I  zoea.  Note  dense  pigmentation  at  the  base  of  the  ommatidia,  and  well-developed  neuropiles  of  the 
lamina  ganglionaris  (LG).  medulla  externa  (ME),  and  the  brain  (*).  (c,  d)  Higher  magnifications  of  sinus 
glands  corresponding  to  rectangles  in  a.  b.  (e)  Cross-sectioned  MIH-IR  axons  (inset  enlarged  from  the  rectangle). 
(f)  Two  MIH-IR  perikarya  in  an  anterior  dorsal  cell  group  of  the  left  eyestalk  ganglia  (inset  enlarged  from 
the  rectangle),  (g)  MIH-IR  axon  profiles  in  the  sinus  gland  (rectangle)  of  a  stage  II  zoea  adjacent  to  the  ME 
and  large  hemolymph  spaces.  Note  stalk  formation  of  the  eye  at  this  stage,  (i)  Cross-sectioned  MIH-IR  axons 
in  the  medulla  terminalis.  (k)  Three  clustered  MIH-IR  penkarya  in  an  anterior  dorsal  position  of  the  pre- 
sumptive X-organ  cell  group.  Note  well-developed  ganglia  and  neuropiles  in  the  eye.  (h,  j,  I)  Higher  mag- 
nifications of  rectangles  outlined  in  g,  i,  k.  Note  axon  profiles  and  putative  terminals  abutting  on  the  surface 
of  the  sinus  gland  (h)  and  dark  PAP  reaction  products  restricted  to  the  cytoplasm  of  the  penkarya  (1)  of 
MIH-IR  neurons. 

Scale  bars:  50  ^m  in  a.  b,  e.  f,  g,  i.  k.  10  /jm  in  c.  d,  h.  j,  1,  and  insets  in  e,  f. 


,      ^r1- 
,c^        I     I'M 


m  ^ 
• 


.-•••• 

!     »1-  '     •     '  '  -•""•      ,     ' 

/Y      '  * 

IIS^        m 


»        wft" 

&  ~'^ 
M]$&.  .-v 


^mm^'-^^\ 


s  .V-i,-.-. 

••>..\ 


\.fo 

yj$! 


g 

•     . 

^-:  '•    •  •  •' 

•$    •      ..'--''  :;'- 

'•,      ,'       •          '  ;' 

-     f-'      ,•'•    '      v..    ./ 

;  -;--: 


'f^^T^U-    ,,-•      •      •/.-  ..     ,    -          -      -  >T,  SB  ",,-. •-,•../,'-.•'  '      /.!'.>    . 

m*[  ^;    :    ••  - :  r  $$$$&  -^t* 
;w,      ••  cM^^P^^  '  " 

\  '-r\--:--'  V-  •"    '^&?Js£s*r 


/      ,  •,  >:  x    v        .   >•     •  \  ".'.'  - 

,  •        .  M    '•  ,_ 

'   ' 


w& 

V   .  .^ 


Figure  2.  Characteristic  structures  of  MIH-immunoreactive  (IR)  neurons  in  stage  III  zoea  (a-f,  left  eye) 
and  stage  IV  zoea  (g-1,  right  eye)  eyes  ofCarcimis  macnas  larvae.  Phase  contrast  micrographs  of  immunostained 
semithin  ( 1  ^m)  transverse  sections.  (Orientation  of  dorsal  parts  of  the  larvae  to  the  tops  of  the  micrographs.) 

68 


MOLT-INHIB1TING  HORMONE  IN  CRAB  LARVAE 


69 


nification  (Fig.  2f)  when  the  black  pigment  granules  could 
be  clearly  resolved  by  phase  contrast  optics. 

M1H  immunoreactivity  was  found  in  all  zoeal  stages 
examined,  including  the  so-called  prezoeal  stage,  which, 
in  view  of  its  brevity  (ca.  30  min),  and  association  with 
hatching,  could  well  be  described  as  an  embryonic  molt. 
(Fig.  la,  c).  In  general,  MIH  immunoreactivity  was  found 
in  structures  similar  to  those  found  in  the  adult,  including 
perikarya  in  a  position  similar  to  the  X-organ  in  adults, 
an  X-organ  sinus  gland  tract,  and  a  sinus  gland  (Figs.  1, 
2).  In  several  preparations  the  sinus  gland  appeared  to  be 
in  close  proximity  to  a  large  hemolymph  vessel  (Figs.  Ih, 
2a,  d).  By  serially  sectioning  through  the  entire  eyestalk, 
a  maximum  of  four  immunopositive  perikarya  of  about 
8-10  ^m  in  diameter  were  observed  in  all  zoeal  stages 
localized  in  a  cluster  of  neuroblasts  in  an  anterior  dorsal 
position  of  the  eyestalk,  with  large  nuclei  and  scarce  cy- 
toplasm (Figs.  If,  k,  1.  2c,  f,  i).  Axonal  projections  were 
found  in  the  medulla  terminalis  of  the  well-developed 
eyestalk  ganglia  in  a  typical  circular  arrangement  of  four 
cross-sectioned  axons  (Figs.  Ij.  2e,  k),  reminiscent  of  the 
axonal  arrangement  in  the  adult  crab.  This  pattern  was 
found  in  all  zoeal  stages.  Despite  exhaustive  investigation, 
the  only  discernable  change  in  the  morphology  of  the 
neurosecretory  structures  was  the  size  of  the  sinus  gland, 
which  appeared  to  increase  in  volume  between  zoea  I  and 
II,  when  the  eye  became  stalked  and  mobile.  Indeed,  it 
was  frequently  difficult  to  observe  the  sinus  gland  in  zoea 
I  due  to  its  small  size,  but  in  zoea  II,  the  sinus  gland  was 
often  the  most  striking  immunopositive  structure  (Fig. 
Ib,  d,  g,  h).  In  control  incubations,  preabsorbtion  of  the 
antiserum  with  2  nmoles  of  MIH  per  jul  of  crude  antiserum 
completely  abolished  immunostaining,  thus  proving  the 
specificity  of  the  immunocytochemical  detection  (results 
not  shown). 

Discussion 

In  the  present  study,  the  location  of  perikarya,  axons, 
and  sinus  gland  terminals  immunopositive  for  MIH  have 


been  demonstrated  in  all  zoeal  instars  ofCarcinus  larvae. 
Surprisingly,  larval  immunopositive  structures  were  to- 
pographically and  morphologically  similar  to  those  found 
in  the  adult  crab.  However,  very  few  (maximum  4)  MIH- 
immunoreactive  perikarya  were  observed  in  any  larval 
stage,  compared  to  the  adult  crab  where  there  are  32-36 
MIH-immunoreactive  perikarya  (Dircksen  et  ai,  1988). 
It  is  likely  that  the  increase  in  number  of  immunopositive 
cells  during  larval  to  juvenile/adult  development  is  due 
to  increased  MIH  gene  expression  rather  than  by  cell  di- 
vision because  neuroblasts  are  generally  considered  to  be 
too  highly  differentiated  to  undergo  further  division.  A 
striking  similarity  of  the  larval  MIH  immunopositive 
structures  to  those  of  the  adult  concerns  the  morphology 
of  the  X-organ  sinus  gland  tract.  In  the  adult,  MIH  im- 
munoreactive  axons  form  a  peripheral  tract  around  the 
central  axon  bundle  containing  crustacean  hyperglycemic 
hormone  (CHH)  immunopositive  axons  (Dircksen  et  ai, 
1988).  Although  we  did  not  determine  CHH  in  the  present 
study,  the  similarity  in  the  arrangement  of  the  four  MIH- 
immunoreactive  axons  around  a  central  tract  was  clearly 
suggestive  of  the  adult  morphology. 

Several  studies  have  reported  the  general  development  of 
neural  systems  in  the  crustacean  eyestalk.  Cells  correspond- 
ing to  the  X-organ  have  been  found  in  the  first  larval  stages 
of  all  species  examined  (Birgits.  Orlamiinder,  1942;  Horn- 
arm.  Pinnotheres.  Pyle,  1943:  Crangon.  Dahl,  1957;  Pota- 
mon.  Matsumoto,  1958;  Palaemonetes,  Hubschman,  1963; 
Palaemon.  Little,  1969,  Bellon-Humbert  et  ai,  1978;  As- 
tacus,  Zielhorst  and  Van  Herp,  1976,  Gorgels-Kallen  and 
Meij,  1985).  With  regard  to  the  development  of  the  sinus 
gland,  for  freshwater  crustaceans,  which  hatch  at  an  ad- 
vanced developmental  stage,  the  sinus  gland  is  present  in 
the  first  larval  stage  (Matsumoto,  1958;  Gorgels-Kallen  and 
Meij,  1985).  In  marine  crustaceans,  which  hatch  at  a  rela- 
tively early  stage  of  development,  and  which  often  undergo 
a  lengthy  planktonic  existence  prior  to  a  dramatic  meta- 
morphosis, all  studies  suggest  that  the  sinus  gland  develops 
(or  can  first  be  observed)  late  in  larval  life,  at  about  the  time 


(a)  MIH-IR  axon  profiles  in  the  sinus  gland  (rectangle)  adjacent  to  the  large  hemolymph  vessel  (*)  of  the 
eyestalk.  (b)  Section  slightly  anterior  to  (a)  showing  the  sinus  gland  (arrowhead)  and  cross-sectioned  MIH- 
IR  axons  (rectangle)  in  the  medulla  terminalis.  (c)  Four  MIH-IR  penkarya  (rectangle)  are  found  in  an 
anterior  dorsal  position  of  the  presumptive  X-organ  cell  group,  (d,  e.  I")  Higher  magnifications  of  rectangles 
outlined  in  a.  b,  c.  MIH-IR  putative  axon  terminals  adjacent  to  the  hemolymph  vessel  (*)  are  found  in  the 
sinus  gland  (d).  Note  also  cross-sectioned  MIH-IR  axons  (e)  in  the  medulla  terminalis  and  strong  immu- 
noreactivity of  three  perikarya  (f,  arrowheads).  Arrows  in  (f)  point  to  dark  pigments  usually  found  in  perineural 
sheaths  of  eyestalk  ganglia,  (g)  MIH-IR  axon  profiles  in  the  sinus  gland  (rectangle)  adjacent  to  the  large 
hemolymph  vessel  (*)  of  the  eyestalk.  (h)  Cross-sectioned  axons  of  the  presumptive  X-organ  sinus  gland 
(XO-SG)  tract  in  the  medulla  terminalis.  (i)  Two  MIH-IR  perikarya  in  the  presumptive  X-organ  cell  group 
in  a  dorsal  anterior  position  of  the  proximal  eyestalk  ganglia,  (j,  k,  1)  Higher  magnification  of  rectangles 
outlined  in  g,  h,  i,  MIH-IR  axon  profiles  and  putative  axon  terminals  abutting  on  the  surface  of  the  sinus 
gland,  (*)  indicates  hemolymph  vessel,  (j),  MIH-IR  axons  in  the  XO-SG  tract  (k)  and  two  strongly  immu- 
nopositive XO  perikarya  (I).  Note  unstained  axons  in  the  center  of  the  XO-SG  tract  (k). 
Scale  bars:  50  ^m  in  a-c,  g-i.  10  j/m  in  d-f,  j-l. 


70 


S.  G.  WEBSTER  AND  H.  DIRCKSON 


of  metamorphosis  (stage  V  Palaemonetes,  Hubschman, 
1963,  Palaemon,  Bellon-Humbert  et  a/.,  1978;  stage  III 
Homarns.  Pyle  1943).  Apart  from  a  report  by  Jaques  ( 1975) 
demonstrating  the  presence  of  a  sinus  gland  in  stage  I  Squilla 
mantis  larvae,  this  paper  reports  the  first  demonstration  of 
a  sinus  gland  in  first  stage  larvae  of  a  marine  decapod  crus- 
tacean, and  is  undoubtedly  due  to  the  great  resolving  power 
of  immunocytochemical  techniques  compared  to  conven- 
tional histochemical  staining  methods.  To  our  knowledge, 
the  only  other  reports  using  immunocytochemical  tech- 
niques to  identify  larval  neurosecretory  structures  are  those 
by  Gorgels-Kallen  and  Meij  (1985),  demonstrating  the  neu- 
rosecretory structures  containing  CHH  immunoreactivity 
in  Astaais  leptodactylns  larvae,  and  Beltz  and  Kravitz  (1987) 
and  Beltz  et  al.  (1990),  demonstrating  proctolin-like  im- 
munoreactivity in  the  CNS  of  larval  Homarns  americanus. 
While  immunocytochemical  evidence  indicates  that 
Carcinus  zoeae  possess  a  M1H  neurosecretory  system, 
which  may  participate  in  the  control  of  larval  molting, 
experiments  involving  eyestalk  ablation  in  several  species 
of  crustacean  larvae  (see  specific  examples  in  Charmantier 
et  al..  1988;  Christiansen,  1988)  have  demonstrated  that, 
in  general,  eyestalk  ablation  is  only  effective  in  accelerating 
proecdysis  and  molting  when  performed  during  the  last 
instar  before  metamorphosis.  Although  the  deficiencies 
of  these  experiments  have  been  commented  upon  by 
Freeman  and  Costlow  ( 1980),  particularly  with  regard  to 
difficulties  in  determining  the  precise  duration  of  instars 
and  the  time  of  initiation  of  proecdysis  in  rapidly  moulting 
larvae,  it  has  been  suggested  (Freeman  et  al.,  1983)  that 
the  larval  molt  cycle  is  not  regulated  by  MIH  until  meta- 
morphosis. However,  studies  demonstrating  that  larval 
ecdysteroid  liters  cycle  in  a  molt-stage-dependent  manner 
in  much  the  same  way  as  adults  (Chang  and  Bruce,  1981; 
Spindler  and  Anger,  1986),  and  a  report  by  Snyder  and 
Chang  (1986),  demonstrating  that  increases  in  proecdysial 
ecdysteroid  titer  induced  by  eyestalk  removal  of  Stage  II 
Homarns  zoeae  can  be  repressed  by  the  injection  of  adult 
sinus  gland  extracts,  strongly  support  the  hypothesis  that 
larval  molting  (or  at  least,  initiation  of  proecdysis)  is  reg- 
ulated by  MIH,  and  the  results  presented  here  would  also 
support  this  hypothesis.  However,  it  should  be  stressed 
that  no  firm  inferences  as  to  the  function  of  the  immu- 
noreactive  MIH  can  yet  be  made;  it  is  not  known  whether 
larval  MIH-immunoreactive  material  is  identical  to  that 
in  adults,  although  the  antiserum  used  displays  a  very 
high  specificity  in  immunodot  assays  (Dircksen  el  al.. 
1988),  RIA,  and  ELISA  (Webster,  unpub.),  or  whether  it 
is  released  during  the  zoeal  stages.  Although  in  vivo  ex- 
periments involving  injection  of  MIH  or  sinus  gland  ex- 
tracts into  zoeal  larvae  and  subsequent  monitoring  of 
proecdysis  or  instar  length  would  undoubtedly  strengthen 
hypotheses  concerning  larval  molt  control,  the  small  size 
of  most  crab  zoeae  argues  against  the  success  of  such  ex- 


periments in  crab  larvae.  A  further  problem,  which  re- 
mains unresolved,  concerns  the  increase  in  number  of 
immunoreactive  perikarya  between  the  last  zoeal  stage 
and  the  adult.  It  is  possible  that  this  transition  occurs 
during  metamorphosis  (a  phenomenon  we  could  not  elu- 
cidate due  to  difficulties  in  achieving  adequate  fixation  of 
megalopae  and  first  crab  stages).  If  the  MIH  secretory 
system  became  synthetically  active  at  this  time,  and  stored 
MIH  was  released,  then  previous  observations  regarding 
the  failure  to  accelerate  molting  in  zoeal  larvae,  and  the 
appearance  of  the  sinus  gland  as  a  structure  stainable  by 
conventional  histochemical  methods  prior  to  metamor- 
phosis, could  be  reconciled  with  the  model  of  molt  control 
suggested  by  Freeman  et  al.  (1983). 


Acknowledgments 

We  are  grateful  to  Mr.  M.  Budd,  School  of  Ocean  Sci- 
ences, Menai  Bridge,  UK,  for  culturing  the  phytoplankton 
and  rotifers  used  in  this  study,  and  for  much  useful  advice 
concerning  larval  rearing  techniques.  This  work  was  sup- 
ported by  a  Royal  Society  University  Research  Fellowship 
(S.G.W.).  Financial  support  from  the  British  Council  for 
travel  to  Bangor  (H.D.)  is  gratefully  acknowledged. 


Literature  Cited 

Bellon-llumbert,    C,    M.    J.    P.    Thijssen,    and    F.    Van    Herp. 

1978.  Development,  location  and  relocation  of  sensory  and  neu- 
rosecretory sites  in  the  eyestalks  during  the  larval  and  postlarval  life 
of  Palaemon  .terrains  (Pennant).  J.  Mar.  Biol.  Assoc.  U.K.  58:  851- 
868. 

Beltz,  B.  S.,  and  E.  A.  Kravitz.  1987.  Physiological  identification,  mor- 
phological analysis  and  development  of  identified  serotonin-proctolin 
containing  neurons  in  the  lobster  ventral  nerve  cord.  J.  Neurosci.  7: 
533-546. 

Beltz,  B.  S.,  M.  Pontes,  S.  M.  Helluy,  and  E.  A.  Kravitz.  1990.  Patterns 
of  appearance  of  serotonin  and  proctolin  immunoreactivities  in  the 
developing  nervous  system  of  the  American  lobster.  /  Neurobiol. 
21:521-542. 

C'hang,  E.  S.,  and  M.  J.  Bruce.  1981.  Ecdysteroid  liters  of  larval  lobsters. 
Com/1  Biochein.  Physiol.  70A:  239-241. 

Charmantier,  G.,  M.  Charmantier-Daures,  and  D.  E.  Aiken. 
1988.  Larval  development  and  metamorphosis  of  the  American 
lobster  Homarus  americanus  (Crustacea,  Decapoda):  effect  of  eyestalk 
ablation  and  juvenile  hormone  injection.  Gen.  Comp.  Endocrinol. 
70:  319-333. 

Christiansen,  M.  E.  1988.  Hormonal  processes  in  decapod  crustacean 
larvae.  Symp.  Zool.  Soc.  Loud.  59:  46-68. 

Dahl,  E.  1957.  Embryology  of  X-organs  in  Crangon  allmanni.  Nature 
179:  482. 

Dircksen,  H.,  C.  A.  Zahnow,  G.  Gaus,  R.  Keller,  K.  R.  Rao,  and  J.  P. 
Riehm.  1987.  The  ultrastructure  of  nerve  endings  containing  pig- 
ment dispersing  hormone  (PDH)  in  crustacean  sinus  glands:  iden- 
tification by  an  antiserum  against  synthetic  PDH.  Cell  Tiss.  Res. 
250:  377-387. 

Dircksen,  H.,  S.  G.  Webster,  and  R.  Keller.  1988.  Immunocytochemical 
demonstration  of  the  neurosecretory  systems  containing  putative 


MOLT-INHIBITING   HORMONE  IN  CRAB  LARVAE 


71 


molt-inhihiting  hormone  and  hyperglycemic  hormone  in  the  eyestalk 

of  hrachyuran  crustaceans.  Cell  Tiss.  Res  251:  3-12. 
Freeman,  J.  A.,  and  J.  D.  Costlow.  1980.     The  molt  cycle  and  its  hor- 
monal control  in  Rhithmpanopeus  liarnsi  larvae.  Dev  Biol  74: 479- 

485. 
Freeman,  J.  A.,T.  L.  West,  and  J.  D.Costkm.  1983.     Postlarval  growth 

in  juvenile  Rlulhropanopeus  harnsii.  Biol  Bull  165:409-415. 
Gorgels-Kallen,  J.  L.,  and  J.  T.  A.  Meij.  1985.     Immunocytochemical 

study  of  the  hyperglycemic  hormone  (CHH)-producing  system  in 

the  eyestalk  of  the  crayfish  Astacus  leptodactylus  during  larval  and 

postlarval  development.  J  Morphol.  185:  155-163. 
Hubschman,  J.  II.  1963.     Development  and  function  of  neurosecretory 

sites  in  the  eyestalks  of  larval  Palaemoneles  (Decapoda,  Natantia). 

Biol.  Bull.  125:96-113. 
Jaques,  F.  1975.     Decouverte  de  la  glande  du  sinus  chez  la  larve  de 

Si/iii/lu  mantis  (stade  I)  (Crustace,  Stomatopodes).  Ultrastructure. 

C.  R  Acad.  Sa.  (D)  (Paris)  280:  1575-1577. 
Lachaise,  F.,  M.  Hubert,  S.  G.  Webster,  and  R.  Lafont.  1988.     Effect 

of  molt-inhibiting  hormone  on  ketodiol  conversion  by  crab  Y-organs. 

J  Insect  Physiol.  34:  557-562. 
Little,  G.  1969.     The  larval  development  of  the  shrimp.  Palaemon  mac- 

rodaaylus  Rathbun,  reared  in  the  laboratory,  and  the  effect  of  eyestalk 

extirpation  on  development.  Crustaceana  17:  69-87. 
Matsumoto,  K.  1958.     Morphological  studies  on  the  neurosecretion  in 

crabs.  Biol  J.  Okayama  L'nir.  (Japan)  4:  103-176. 
Orlamunder,  J.  1942.  Zur  Entwicklung  und  Formbildung  des  Birgiix 

lalro  L.  mil  besonderer  Beriacksichtigung  des  X-Organs.  Z    Wiss. 

Zool.  155:280-316. 
Pyle,  R.  W.  1943.     The  histogenesis  and  cyclic  phenomena  of  the  sinus 

gland  and  X-organ  in  Crustacea.  Biol.  Bull  85:  87-102. 
Rice,  A.  L.,  and  R.  W.  Ingle.  1975.     The  larval  development  ofCarcinus 

maenax (L.)  and  C  mediterraneus  Czerniavsky  (Crustacea.  Brachyura. 


Portumdae)  reared  in  the  laboratory.  Bull  Bril  Mus.  (Not.  Hist.)  28: 
103-119. 

Skinner,  D.  M.  1985.  Interacting  factors  in  the  control  of  the  crustacean 
molt  cycle.  Am.  Zool  25:  275-284. 

Snyder,  M.  J.,  and  E.  S.  Chang.  1986.  Effects  of  sinus  gland  extracts 
on  larval  molting  and  ecdysteroid  liters  of  the  American  lobster. 
Homants  amcricanus.  Biol  Bull  170:  244-254. 

Spindler,  K.  D.,  and  K.  Anger.  1986.  Ecdysteroid  levels  during  the 
larval  development  of  the  spider  crab  Hya.i  araneus.  Gen.  Camp. 
Endoa-moL  64:  122-128. 

Spurr,  A.  R.  1969.  A  low-viscosity  epoxy  resin  embedding  medium 
for  electron  microscopy.  J.  Ultrastruc.  Res.  26:  31-43. 

Watson,  R.  D.,  E.  Spaziani,  and  W.  E.  Bollenbacher.  1989.  Regulation 
of  ecdysone  synthesis  in  insects  and  crustaceans:  a  comparison.  Pp. 
188-203  in  Ecdysone.  From  Chemistry  10  Mode  of  Action,  J.  Kool- 
man.  ed.  Georg  Thieme  Verlag.  Stuttgart. 

Webster,  S.  G.  1986.  Neurohormonal  control  of  ecdysteroid  biosyn- 
thesis by  Carcinus  maenas  Y-organs  in  vitro  and  preliminary  char- 
acterization of  the  putative  moult-inhibiting  hormone  (MIH).  Gen. 
Comp  Endocrinol.  61:  237-247. 

Webster.  S.  G.,  and  R.  Keller.  1986.  Purification,  characterization  and 
amino  acid  composition  of  the  putative  moult-inhibiting  hormone 
(MIH)  ofCarcinin  >iiiicna\  (Crustacea,  Decapoda).  /  Comp.  Physiol 
B  156:  617-624. 

Webster,  S.  G.,  and  R.  Keller.  1988.  Physiology  and  biochemistry  of 
crustacean  neurohormonai  peptides.  Pp.  173-196  in  Neurohormones 
in  Invertebrates,  M.  C.  Thorndyke  and  G.  J.  Goldsworthy.  eds.  Cam- 
bridge LJniversity  Press. 

Zielhorst,  A.  J.  A.  G.,  and  F.  Van  llerp.  1976.  Developpement  du 
systeme  neurosecreteur  du  pedoncle  oculaire  des  larves  d'  Astacus 
leptodactylus  salinus  Nordmann  (Crustacea,  Decapoda,  Reptantia): 
Microscopie  photonique.  C  R  Acad.  Sci  (D)  (Paris)  283:  1755-1758. 


Reference:  Bin/.  Bull.  180:  72-80.  (February,  1991) 


The  First  Historical  Extinction  of  a  Marine 

Invertebrate  in  an  Ocean  Basin:  The  Demise 

of  the  Eelgrass  Limpet  Lottia  alveus 


JAMES  T.  CARLTON1,  GEERAT  J.  VERMEIJ2,  DAVID  R.  LINDBERG3, 
DEBBY  A.  CARLTON1,  AND  ELIZABETH  C.  DUDLEY4 

^Maritime  Studies  Program,  Williams  College — Mystic  Seaport,  Mystic,  Connecticut  06355; 

^Department  of  Geology,  University  of  California,  Davis,  California  95616; 3 'Museum  of 

Paleontology,  University  of  California,  Berkeley,  California  94720;  and4  Department  of  Zoology, 

University  of  Maryland,  College  Park.  Maryland  20742 


Abstract.  Lottia  alveus,  a  gastropod  limpet  once  found 
only  on  the  blades  of  the  eelgrass  Zostera  marina  from 
Labrador  to  New  York  in  the  western  Atlantic  Ocean,  is 
the  first  marine  invertebrate  known  to  have  become  ex- 
tinct in  an  ocean  basin  in  historical  time.  The  last  known 
specimens  were  collected  in  1929,  immediately  prior  to 
the  catastrophic  decline  of  Zostera  in  the  early  1930s  in 
the  North  Atlantic  Ocean.  The  brackish  water  refugium 
ofZoslera  throughout  the  decline  was  apparently  outside 
of  this  gastropod's  physiological  range,  and  the  limpet 
became  extinct.  Few  marine  invertebrates  have  habits  as 
specialized  and  ranges  and  tolerances  as  narrow  as  did  L. 
alveus.  The  fact  that  most  marine  invertebrates  have  large 
effective  population  sizes  may  account  for  their  relative 
invulnerability  to  extinction. 

Introduction 

There  are  no  reports  of  the  post-Pleistocene  extinction 
of  any  marine  invertebrate,  in  spite  of  the  fact  that 
hundreds  of  terrestrial  and  freshwater  species  of  animals 
and  plants  have  become  extinct  as  human  activity  has 
increased  around  the  world  (Martin  and  Klein,  1984: 
Vermeij,  1986;  McNeely  et  a/..  1990).  This  is  perhaps 
even  more  remarkable  given  the  widespread  perception 
that  many  marine  invertebrate  species  have  suffered  ex- 
tensive decimation  and  that  a  number  of  them  are  on 
endangered  species  lists  (for  example,  Gee  and  Wilson, 
1981;  Franz,  1982;  Wells  et  ai,  1983;  Wicksten,  1984). 

Received  18  July  1990;  accepted  24  November  1990. 


We  report  here  the  first  historical  extinction  of  a  marine 
invertebrate  from  an  ocean  basin.  The  limpet  Lottia  alveus 
(Fig.  1),  a  once  abundant  stenotopic  species  that  ranged 
from  southern  Labrador  to  Long  Island  Sound  and  lived 
only  on  the  blades  of  the  eelgrass  Zostera  marina  (Conrad, 
1831;Couthouy,  1839;  Gould  and  Binney,  1870),  is  now 
extinct  in  the  Atlantic  Ocean.  Here  we  consider  the  evi- 
dence for  this  conclusion  and  suggest  why  this  extinction 
occurred. 


Materials  and  Methods 


Field  studies 


Eelgrass  populations  were  searched  specifically  for  lim- 
pets in  the  following  locations:  Cape  Cod,  Massachusetts, 
between  1979  and  1982;  along  the  eastern  Connecticut 
shore  (Fishers  Island  and  Long  Island  Sounds)  between 
1982-1987  and  1989-1990,  and  at  Vinalhaven  (25  km 
east  of  Rockland).  central  Maine  in  1984  (J.T.C.  and 
D.A.C.);  at  Boothbay  Harbor  (45  km  southwest  of  Rock- 
land),  central  Maine  in  1971,  and  in  Newfoundland  (Come 
by  Chance,  in  Placentia  Bay,  and  at  Norris  Point,  Bonne 
Bay,  in  the  Gulf  of  St.  Lawrence)  in  1990  (G.J.V.).  We 
contacted  biologists  who  are  familiar  with  the  common 
Atlantic  limpet  Tectura  testudinalis  (=  Acmaea  testudi- 
nalis)  and  who  have  sampled  Zostera  epiphytes  in  Quebec 
(Rimouski),  Nova  Scotia  (Halifax),  Maine,  Massachusetts, 
Rhode  Island,  and  Connecticut.  Since  1965,  L.  alveus  has 
been  searched  for  without  success  in  south-central  Nova 
Scotia,  and  in  Labrador  and  Newfoundland  (D.  Davis  and 
R.  Noseworthy,  pers.  comm.,  respectively).  We  examined 


72 


EXTINCT  MARINE  INVERTEBRATE 


73 


Figure  I .  Dorsal  and  lateral  views  of  the  extinct  Atlantic  limpet  Loltia 
alreiis  (a  pre-1900  specimen  from  Massachusetts.  ANSP  39044,  1.6X). 
The  laterally  compressed  shell  of  this  limpet  precisely  fitted  the  narrow 
blade  of  the  eelgrass  Zostera  marina. 


all  published  records(from  1831  to  1989)  of  shallow-water 
marine  mollusks  and  eelgrass  biota  from  the  Arctic  Ocean 
to  the  central  Atlantic  coast  of  the  United  States. 

Museum  studies 

We  examined  14  museum  malacological  collections  in 
search  of  specimens  of  L.  alveus.  For  systematic  purposes 
and  for  trophic  analyses,  we  studied  radulae  of  alcohol 
preserved  and  rehydrated  specimens  of  L.  alveus.  as  well 
as  radulae  of  L.  alveus  parallela  and  illustrations  of  the 
radula  of  L.  alveus  angusta. 

These  collections  are  located  in  the  following  museums 
(abbreviations  are  given  for  museums  cited  later  in  the 
text):  Academy  of  Natural  Sciences,  Philadelphia  (ANSP); 
American  Museum  of  Natural  History,  New  York;  British 
Columbia  Provincial  Museum,  Victoria  (BCPM);  Cali- 
fornia Academy  of  Sciences,  San  Francisco;  Los  Angeles 
County  Museum  of  Natural  History;  Museum  of  Com- 
parative Zoology,  Harvard  University  (MCZ);  Museum 
of  Paleontology,  University  of  California,  Berkeley;  Na- 
tional Museums  of  Canada,  Ottawa;  New  York  State  Mu- 
seum, Albany  (NYSM);  Natural  History  Museum,  Lon- 
don [formerly  British  Museum  (Natural  History)];  Nova 
Scotia  Museum,  Halifax;  Santa  Barbara  Museum  of  Nat- 
ural History.  Santa  Barbara,  California;  United  States 
Museum  of  Natural  History,  Smithsonian  Institution. 
Washington,  DC  (USNM);  University  of  Alaska  Museum, 
Fairbanks  (U AM). 

In  addition,  a  number  of  major  United  States  herbar- 
ium collections  of  the  eelgrass  Zostera  marina  from  North 
America  were  examined  in  an  independent  study  on  eel- 
grass  wasting  diseases  by  F.  Short,  who  has  provided  us 
with  his  records  of  dried  limpets  found  on  herbarium 
sheets. 


Results 

Systematics  and  biogeography 

The  limpet  Lot  tin  alveus  (Conrad)  was  described  in 
1831  from  Massachusetts.  It  is  more  commonly  known 
as  Acmaea  alveus  or  Co/lisella  alveus.  We  follow  the  no- 
menclatural  revision  of  Lindberg  (1986)  in  referring  this 
species  to  Lottia.  Two  situations  led  to  the  previously 
overlooked  history  of  this  limpet  in  the  North  Atlantic 
Ocean.  First,  there  was  a  persistent  belief  that  L.  alveus 
was  an  ecotype  of  the  rocky  intertidal  limpet  Tectura  tes- 
tui/ina/is(Mu\\cT,  1776)  (Dall,  1871;  Johnson,  1928;  Ab- 
bott, 1974),  and  that  it  was  thus  not  a  separate  species. 
Second,  there  are  continued  reports  of  its  presumed  pres- 
ence on  the  Atlantic  coast  in  molluscan  checklists  and 
books  (for  example,  Abbott,  1954,  1974;  Emerson  and 
Jacobson,  1976). 

However,  as  Jackson  (1907)  and  Morse  (1910,  1921) 
clearly  demonstrated,  L.  alveus  is  distinct  from  T.  testu- 
dinalis  in  anatomy,  behavior,  shell  shape,  sculpture,  and 
color.  Morse  (1910)  noted  that  shells  of  the  two  species 
could  be  distinguished  at  "a  millimeter  or  more"  in  length, 
by  sculpture,  apex  shape,  and  color.  McLean  (1966)  fur- 
ther noted  that  L.  a/veux  was  not  a  form  of  T.  tesludinalis 
that  had  settled  on  eelgrass  blades,  as  both  T.  testudina/is 
and  the  eelgrass  Zostera  marina  are  common  in  European 
waters,  where  L.  alveus  does  not  occur.  William  Healey 
Dall,  whose  opinion  was  widely  regarded  by  contemporary 
malacologists,  also  concluded,  in  a  reversal  of  his  earlier 
belief  (Dall,  1871),  that  L.  alveus  was  "a  good  species" 
(Sumner  et  a/..  1913).  He  was  apparently  influenced  by 
the  findings  of  Jackson  (and  perhaps  Morse),  but  his 
opinion  apparently  did  not  reach  the  general  malacologi- 
cal community. 

McLean  (1966)  and  Lindberg  (1986)  have  shown  that 
L.  alveus  and  T.  testudinalis  are  properly  placed  in  dif- 
ferent genera.  The  genus  Lottia  possesses  a  single  pair  of 
reduced  marginal  teeth  (uncini)  that  are  present  at  the 
posterior  end  of  the  ribbon  segment.  The  genus  Tectura 
lacks  these  marginal  teeth  on  the  radula.  Lindberg  (1981, 
1 986,  1 988)  discusses  the  phylogenetic  importance  of  these 
radular  characters  in  diagnosing  limpet  genera.  Jackson 
( 1907)  detailed  other  differences  between  the  radulae  of 
the  two  species,  although  he  failed  to  illustrate  the  uncini. 

Lottia  alveus  originated  in  the  North  Pacific  Ocean 
from  an  ancestral  lineage  represented  in  the  Mio-Pliocene 
of  Japan  by  Lottia  august  itesta  (Yokoyama,  1926)  (Yo- 
koyama,  1926;  Kotaka  and  Ogasawara,  1974;  D.R.L.,  in 
prep.).  The  Western  North  Pacific  Ocean  is  also  consid- 
ered the  center  of  origin  in  the  Tertiary  of  Zostera 
(McRoy,  1968;  den  Hartog,  1970).  Both  L.  alveus  and 
Zostera  invaded  the  North  Atlantic  Ocean  through  the 
Bering  Strait  and  the  Arctic  Ocean  in  the  late  Tertiary, 


74 


J.  T.  CARLTON  ET  AL. 


as  did  numerous  other  marine  organisms  (Durham  and 
MacNeil,  1967;  G.J.V.,  in  prep.). 

Pleistocene  glaciation  subsequently  created  three  allo- 
patric  subspecies:  Lotlia  alveus  parallela  (Dall,  1914)  in 
the  Northeast  Pacific,  Lottia  alveus  angusta  (Moskalev, 
1967)  in  the  Northwest  Pacific,  and  Lottia  alveus  alveus 
(hereafter.  L  alveus)  in  the  Northwest  Atlantic.  The  three 
subspecies  are  distinguished  on  the  basis  of  external  mor- 
phology and  radulae  (D.R.L.,  in  prep.).  In  addition,  the 
Atlantic  subspecies  had  markedly  less  variation  in  color 
and  shell  pattern  than  Pacific  populations,  and  also  pos- 
sessed a  widespread  radular  abnormality  (an  extra  first 
lateral  tooth  on  the  left  side  of  the  radula)  absent  in  Pacific 
individuals.  These  characteristics  in  the  Atlantic  subspe- 
cies suggest  a  founder  effect.  Mitochondria!  DNA  analysis 
(of  the  extant  North  Pacific  populations  and  of  preserved 
material  of  the  North  Atlantic  populations)  may  aid  in 
resolving  whether  these  three  taxa  should  be  treated  as 
full  species. 

Lottia  alveus  parallela  occurs  only  on  Zostera  between 
Kazuna  Bay,  Cook  Inlet  (60°  North  Latitude)  in  southern 
Alaska  (UAM,  N.  Foster  collections,  1975)  and  Smith's 
Inlet  in  Queen  Charlotte  Sound,  British  Columbia  (51° 
North  Latitude)  (BCPM,  late  nineteenth  century  speci- 
mens). Dall  (1921)  cites  a  southern  Pacific  coast  limit  of 
L.  a.  parallela  as  Victoria,  British  Columbia  (48°  North 
Latitude),  but  the  specimen  lot  in  BCPM  upon  which  this 
record  is  apparently  based  indicates  that  the  material  may 
also  have  been  collected  at  Skidegate  Inlet,  on  the  east 
coast  of  Queen  Charlotte  Island.  Burch  (1946)  cites  what 
appears  to  be  an  independent  Victoria  record,  but  without 
data,  and  we  have  been  unable  to  locate  supporting  ma- 
terial. We  know  of  no  formal  searches  in  Alaska  or  British 
Columbia  that  have  attempted  to  establish  the  exact  dis- 
tribution of  L.  a.  parallela.  Lottia  a.  angusta  has  been 
recorded  only  from  Sakhalin  Island,  Sea  of  Japan  (46° 
North  Latitude),  on  Zostera  (Moskalev,  1967). 

Lottia  alveus  was  known  as  far  west  (south)  on  the  At- 
lantic coast  as  Long  Island  Sound,  where  it  was  recorded 
from  New  York  by  De  Kay  ( 1 843)  and  Letson  ( 1 905)  (see 
also  Table  I,  herein)  and  from  Stratford,  Connecticut  by 
Linsley  ( 1 845).  It  occurred  as  far  east  (north)  as  Egg  Har- 
bor, Labrador  (USNM,  O.  Bryant  collections,  1908) 
(Fig.  2). 

The  last  known  populations 

No  eelgrass  limpets  have  been  collected  in  the  Atlantic 
Ocean  since  1929  (Table  I).  The  previous  known  range 
of  this  limpet  (Labrador  to  New  York)  has  been  searched 
thoroughly  by  us  and  others.  Given  the  planktotrophic 
larva  that  lottiid  limpets  possess  (Lindberg,  1981),  and 
the  now  widespread  occurrence  and  availability  of  Zostera 
as  a  habitat,  we  do  not  believe  that  there  are  refugial. 


isolated  "pockets"  of  this  limpet  in  remote  coves,  offshore 
islands,  or  similar  sites. 

Two  live  collected  USNM  specimens  ( 1 3.0  and  9.3  mm 
in  length)  of  L.  alveus  bear  a  label  indicating  the  place  of 
collection  as  Cape  Ann.  Massachusetts  (50  km  northeast 
of  Boston)  and  a  date  of  14  July  1953.  We  have  excluded 
this  record  from  Table  I  for  the  following  reasons.  In  con- 
trast to  the  records  listed  in  Table  I,  we  have  been  unable 
to  verify  that  this  is  the  date  of  collection  (for  example, 
by  other  species  collected  at  the  same  time  and  place  by 
the  same  collector,  by  knowledge  of  the  collector's  specific 
activities  at  the  time  and  place  of  collection,  and  so  forth). 
The  collector  (J.  A.  Weber)  specialized  (as  a  hobby)  in 
collecting  gastropod  radulae,  and  obtained  material  from 
many  sources.  Thus  he  may.  for  example,  have  obtained 
preserved  or  dried  material  of  this  limpet  from  another 
shell  collector  (Weber  made  a  long  trip  up  the  coast  in 
1953,  visiting  shell  collectors  and  collecting  specimens). 
The  specimens  were  received  at  the  Smithsonian  Insti- 
tution in  1966;  while  the  Latin  name  and  location  are 
part  of  the  original  writing,  the  date  has  been  added  in 
black  ink  at  a  later  time.  Dexter  (1968)  systematically 
sampled  the  mollusks  at  five  widely  separated  stations  at 
Cape  Ann  from  1933  to  1937  and  from  1956  to  1961, 
and  in  many  intervening  years  through  1967.  While  find- 
ing many  uncommon  and  rare  species,  he  never  found 
L.  alveus  (R.  Dexter,  pers.  comm.,  1990).  Dexter  specif- 
ically examined  the  mollusks  on  eelgrass  blades  at  Cape 
Ann  in  1949  (Dexter,  1950).  again  without  finding  L.  al- 
veus. Dexter  was  also  at  Cape  Ann  in  July  1953,  where 
he  did  not  find  L.  alveus  in  informal  surveys  of  the  eel- 
grass,  nor  did  he  meet  Weber  there  (R.  Dexter,  pers. 
comm..  1990). 

We  do  not  discount  this  record  because  it  occurs  after 
1929,  nor  because  it  does  not  fit  our  view  of  the  timing 
of  the  extinction  of  this  mollusk.  The  possible  persistence 
of  L.  alveus  until  the  early  1950s  does  not  alter  our  con- 
clusion that  this  limpet  is  extinct.  Many  extinctions  are 
characterized  by  a  lengthy  and  slow  decline  of  a  species, 
rather  than  by  the  precipitous  disappearance  documented 
here.  Thus,  one  scenario  for  the  demise  of  L.  alveiis  would 
have  been  a  catastrophic  bottleneck  followed  by  the  even- 
tual disappearance  of  the  last  remnant  populations  over 
subsequent  decades.  Rather,  we  reject  this  record  because 
decades  of  sampling  and  collecting  mollusks  specifically 
at  Cape  Ann,  and  in  the  Boston  area  in  general,  before 
and  after  1953  have  failed  to  discover  this  limpet.  It  is 
not  infrequent  to  find  on  museum  labels  transmittal  dates, 
exchange  dates,  and  cataloging  dates,  and  we  thus  suggest, 
pending  other  confirmation,  that  "1953"  is  one  of  these 
dates-of-record. 

The  last  verifiable  report  of  living  eelgrass  limpets  in 
the  Atlantic  Ocean  is  that  of  Proctor  (1933).  Collecting 
in  1 929  ( fide  Johnson,  1 929)  at  Bar  Harbor  on  Mt.  Desert 


EXTINCT  MARINE  INVERTEBRATE 


75 


Long  lilond 
Sound 


Figure  2.     Former  populations  (dots)  of  the  limpet  Lotlia  alveus  in  the  Northwest  Atlantic  Ocean.  Triangles 
represent  other  localities  mentioned  in  text. 


Island  on  the  northeastern  Maine  coast.  Proctor  reported 
that  "One  may  go  to  the  Narrows  [near  Bar  Harbor]  at 
low  tide  today  and  find  .  .  .  thousands  of  individuals 
readily  accessible  .  .  ."  Proctor  believed  (evidently  on  the 
basis  of  shell  color  and  shape)  that  L.  alveus  and  T.  tes- 
tudinalis  were  identical  species.  Their  abundance  may 
have  been  a  source  of  his  confusion.  It  is  possible  that  he 
found  dislodged  L.  alveus  individuals  upon  rocks  and  er- 
rant T.  testudinalis  individuals  on  eelgrass  blades.  There 
are  reports  ofL.  alveus  from  rocks  (Stimpson,  1851;  Jack- 
son, 1907;  Morse,  1910)  that  Morse  (19 10)  believed  to  be 
the  result  of  specimens  detached  by  waves  and  storms. 
Lottia  alveus  was  on  occasion  also  found  on  other  sub- 
strates. There  is,  for  example,  a  specimen  (MCZ)  collected 
in  1 897  at  Isle  au  Haul,  Maine,  attached  to  the  periwinkle 
Littorina  liltorea  (Linnaeus,  1758),  bearing  the  label, 
"living  thus  on  this  specimen  of  L.  lillorea  which  was  on 


(a)  float .  .  .  in  bed  of  eelgrass."  The  typically  rock-dwell- 
ing limpet  Lottia  pelta  (Rathke,  1833)  can  be  found  oc- 
casionally in  California  on  the  blades  of  the  surfgrass 
Phyllospadix  when  dense  stands  of  the  latter  overlap  in- 
tertidal  rocks  (J.T.C.,  pers.  observ.) 

Reconstruction  of  the  biology  o/"Lottia  alveus 

The  morphology,  anatomy,  habitat,  and  collection  rec- 
ords of  Lottia  alveus  permit  a  partial  reconstruction  of 
the  biology  and  natural  history  of  this  extinct  Atlantic 
species.  There  are  no  studies  of  the  extant  subspecies  in 
the  North  Pacific  Ocean. 

Abundance 

As  with  many  now  uncommon  animals  and  plants  re- 
ported as  "common"  or  "abundant"  in  the  nineteenth 


76 


J.  T.  CARLTON  ET  AL 


century,  there  are  no  quantitative  analyses  of  the  popu- 
lation size  or  structure  of  Lottia  alveus.  However,  a  sense 
of  the  abundance  of  this  eelgrass  limpet  can  be  gleaned 
from  the  literature  (Table  II).  It  is  clear  that  this  limpet 
was  sufficiently  common  throughout  much  of  northern 
New  England  that  it  could  be  collected  "on  demand"  be- 
tween the  1860s  and  the  late  1920s.  While  workers  con- 
tinued to  refer  to  L.  alveus  in  later  years  [for  example. 
Miner's  (1950)  statement,  "found  abundantly  on  eel- 
grass"],  it  is  clear  that  these  are  references  to  older  liter- 
ature and  collections. 

Trophic  ecology 

The  radula  of  L.  alveus  was  illustrated  by  Jackson 
(1907).  We  find  it  to  be  an  accurate  figure,  with  the  ex- 
ception of  the  missing  uncini.  Analysis  of  the  radular 
morphology  of  L.  alveus  indicates  that  it  was  a  trophic 
specialist,  feeding  upon  the  epithelial  cells  of  the  eelgrass, 
rather  than  upon  epiphytic  diatoms  and  algae.  The  radula 
of  all  alveus  subspecies  has  broad,  straight  cutting  edges 
on  its  first  and  second  lateral  teeth.  It  is  analogous  to  the 
radula  of  the  Northeast  Pacific  Ocean  stenotopic  surfgrass 
(Phyllospadix)  limpet  Tectwa  paleacea  (Gould,  1853), 
which  eats  only  the  epithelial  cells  of  that  grass  (Fishlyn 
and  Phillips,  1980).  With  the  exceptions  of  specimens  that 
presumably  wandered  offor  were  dislodged  from  eelgrass, 
all  reliable  literature  reports  and  museum  material  indicate 
that  Lottia  alveus  was  restricted  to,  and  by  our  analysis 
ate  only,  the  eelgrass  Zostera  marina.  We  predict  that  the 
extant  subspecies  in  the  North  Pacific  feed  upon  the  ep- 
ithelial cells  of  Zostera. 

Distributional  ecology 

We  have  studied  all  reported  localities  (including  con- 
sideration of  their  probable  nineteenth  century  shoreline 


Table  I 

Final  records  of  the  limpet  Lottia  alveus  in  the  Atlantic  Ocean 


Table  II 

Record*,  i if  ihf  abundance  of  the  limpet  Lottia  alveus  on  the  Atlantic 
coast  of  North  America 


Locality 

Last  known 
collection 

Reference 

New  York:  Long  Island: 

Noyack  Bay 

1926 

(1) 

Massachusetts:  Boston  region 

1921 

(2) 

Maine:  Rockland 

1922 

(3) 

Maine:  Mt.  Desert  Island 

1929 

(4) 

New  Brunswick:  Bay  of 

Fundy:  Grand  Manan 

1920 

(5) 

Quebec:  Saguenay  County: 

Sept-lies 

1925 

(6) 

References:  ( 1 )  NYSM,  R.  C.  Latham,  collector;  (2)  Thompson,  1921; 
(3)  Lermond,  1922;  (4)  Johnson,  1929;  (5)  ANSP.  H.  S.  Colton,  collector; 
(6)  ANSP,  on  Zostera  herbarium  sheet. 


Locality  and  date 


Remarks 


Reference 


"New  England", 

"Found  abundantly 

Gould  and  Binney. 

1860s 

on  the  eel-grass" 

1870 

Grand  Manan 

"very  abundant  on 

Ganong,  1890 

Island.  Bay  of 

eel-grass  at  low 

Fundy.  1890 

water" 

Isle  au  Haul,  Maine. 

[>  1000  specimens  in 

MCZ 

1893-1847 

many  lots] 

North  Haven,  Maine 

"very  common  on 

Jackson,  1908 

[25  km  east  of 

Zostera  marina" 

Rockland],  1908 

"Maine",  1909 

"very  common  all 

Lermond.  1909 

Boston  region,  1910 


Rockland,  Maine, 
1922 


Mt.  Desert  Island, 
Maine,  1929 


along  the  coast, 

on  eel  grass  and 

occasionally  on 

rocks" 
"in  certain  places 

hundreds  may  be 

collected  in  a 

short  time" 
75  specimens  taken 

on  eel  grass  in  one 

afternoon, 

incidental  to  other 

collections 
"thousands  of 

individuals  readily 

accessible"  at  low 

tide 


Morse.  1910 


Lermond,  1922 


Proctor,  1933 


configurations)  for  Lottia  alveus  from  Long  Island  Sound 
to  Labrador  to  reconstruct  aspects  of  the  distributional 
ecology  of  this  limpet. 

Although  no  authors  reported  the  salinity  of  the  water 
in  which  they  collected,  it  appears  that  all  localities  in 
which  L.  alveus  was  collected  were  and  are  characteristic 
of  fully  marine  (32-33%o  or  greater),  rather  than  estuarine, 
habitats.  Of  course  it  is  difficult  to  establish  the  salinity 
of  a  locality  without  actual  records,  but  no  collections 
indicate  that  populations  of  L.  alveus  were  maintained 
on  eelgrass  in  low  salinity  (brackish  water)  sites.  Further 
evidence  may  be  sought  in  the  associated  biota:  the  mol- 
lusks  reported  to  have  been  collected  with  or  in  the  im- 
mediate vicinity  of  L.  alveus  (for  example,  Rathbun,  1 88 1 ; 
Jackson,  1908;  Winkley,  1909;  Thompson.  1921;  Ler- 
mond, 1922)  include  strictly  marine  species,  as  well  as 
euryhaline  species,  but  never  was  the  co-occurring  mol- 
luscan  fauna  (nor  authors'  site  descriptions)  characteristic 
of  strictly  brackish  water. 

We  conclude  that  L.  alveus  was  probably  a  stenohaline 
species  of  open  coastal  waters.  We  predict  that  extant  sub- 


EXTINCT  MARINE  INVERTEBRATE 


77 


species  of  L.  alveus  in  the  North  Pacific  will  he  found  to 
be  stenohaline. 

Discussion 

An  extinction  scenario 

What  factors  led  to  the  extinction  of  this  limpet?  We 
suggest  a  scenario  that  focuses  upon  a  combination  of  the 
stenotopic  habitat  of  this  species  and  its  apparently  narrow 
physiological  range. 

Between  1930  and  1933,  Zostera  precipitously  disap- 
peared from  both  the  eastern  and  western  North  Atlantic 
Ocean  on  a  scale  and  in  geographic  breadth  far  exceeding 
any  previous  historical  declines  (Rasmussen,  1973,  1977). 
The  dramatic  decline  of  this  eelgrass  led  to  extensive  dis- 
ruptions in  neritic  ecosystems,  including  large  reductions 
in  migratory  waterfowl  populations,  loss  of  commercial 
scallop  fisheries,  and  alterations  for  decades  of  nearshore 
soft  sediment  habitats  (Rasmussen,  1977;  Short  et  ai, 
1987).  Until  now,  however,  no  extinctions  have  been  at- 
tributed to  this  decline.  The  primary  cause  of  this  decline 
was  probably  a  "wasting  disease"  caused  by  the  slime  mold 
Labyrinthula  (Muehlstein  et  ai,  1988;  Short  et  ai.  1986, 
1987,  1988).  More  than  90%  of  the  standing  stock  of  Zos- 
tera was  eliminated  with  concomitant  and  often  striking 
changes  in  associated  biota  (Stauffer,  1937;  Dreyer  and 
Castle,  1941). 

Populations  of  Zostera  marina  survived,  however,  in 
low-salinity  refugia  (Short  et  ai,  1986).  As  argued  above, 
we  suggest  that  Lottia  alveus  was  probably  a  stenohaline 
species;  collection  records  indicate  that  it  did  not,  unlike 
Zostera.  extend  into  brackish  waters.  We  speculate  that 
the  presumably  narrow  salinity  range  of  this  limpet  may 
have  prevented  it  from  surviving  on  refugial  eelgrass  pop- 
ulations in  lower  salinity  waters. 

In  contrast,  the  sacoglossan  opisthobranch  Elysia  ca- 
tiilns  Gould,  1 870,  similarly  restricted  to  and  feeding  solely 
upon  eelgrass  (Clark,  1975),  did  not  become  extinct.  This 
small  sea  slug  ranges  from  Boston,  Massachusetts  (John- 
son, 1915)  to  Virginia  (Clark,  1975),  and  probably  south 
to  the  southern  limit  of  Zostera  in  the  Carolinas  (Jensen 
and  Clark,  1983).  Eelgrass  populations  were  similarly 
eliminated  throughout  Elysia's  range,  except,  as  noted, 
in  brackish  water.  We  suggest  that  Elysia  did  not  become 
extinct  because  it  lives  in  salinities  at  least  as  low  as  17%o 
(Marcus,  1972),  and  thus  survived  the  eelgrass  blight  in 
the  estuarine  eelgrass  refugia. 

It  remains  possible,  of  course,  that  factors  other  than 
the  putative  osmoregulatory  abilities  (which  cannot  now 
be  experimentally  determined  for  Atlantic  populations) 
prevented  L.  alveus  from  extending  into  brackish  waters. 
These  factors  could  include  respiratory  intolerance  of  the 
clay-silt  loads  typical  of  estuarine  environments,  or  the 
build-up  of  sediments  or  epiphytes  in  brackish  water  on 


eelgrass  blades  that  may  have  inhibited  the  limpet's  feed- 
ing. For  whatever  reasons,  the  evidence  suggests  that  L. 
alveus  did  not  occur  in  the  upper  bay  environments  in 
which  Zostera  survived. 

Further  evidence  for  this  scenario  is  gained  by  the  ob- 
servation that  other  eelgrass-associated  gastropods  also 
found  refugia  in  other  habitats  or  on  Zostera  in  lower 
salinity  waters.  Snails  typically  found  on  eelgrass  in  New 
England  and  the  middle  Atlantic  coast  include  the  pro- 
sobranchs  Lacuna  vincta  (Montagu,  1803),  Bittium  al- 
ternatum  (Say,  1822),  Bittium  varium  (Pfeiffer,  1840), 
Crepidula  convexa  Say,  1822,  and  Afitrclla  htnata  (Say, 
1826)  (Nagle,  1968;  Marsh,  1973).  None  of  these  is  re- 
stricted to  Zostera,  and  none  became  extinct,  although 
there  are  reports  of  changes  in  microhabitat  and  abun- 
dance following  the  eelgrass  decline  (Dexter,  1962; 
O'Connor,  1972).  Russell-Hunter  and  Tashiro(  1985)  have 
similarly  noted  the  decline  of  the  Z(W/t>ra-associated  in- 
faunal  bivalve  Cumingia  tellinoides  following  the  disap- 
pearance of  eelgrass  beds. 

The  survival  of  Lottia  alveus parallela  and  Lottia  alveus 
angusta  may  result  from  the  fact  that  no  extensive  areas 
of  eelgrass  were  eliminated  in  the  North  Pacific  Ocean 
(den  Hartog,  1987). 

Other  reported  marine  mollusk  extinctions 

Other  marine  mollusks  have  been  reported  as  possibly 
extinct.  We  have  found  no  records  of  any  other  docu- 
mented historical  marine  invertebrate  extinctions. 

A  single  living  specimen  of  the  limpet  "Col/isella"  ed- 
mitchelli  was  collected  in  the  early  1 860s  in  southern  Cal- 
ifornia (Lindberg,  1984).  Nothing  is  known  further  of  the 
Holocene  history  or  habitat  of  this  otherwise  Pleistocene 
species.  The  Caribbean  bivalve  Pholadomya  Candida  was 
believed  extinct  (Runnegar,  1979),  but  it  is  extant  in  waters 
off  Venezuela  (Gibson-Smith  and  Gibson-Smith,  1981). 
The  nudibranch  sea  slug  Doridella  batava,  once  believed 
to  be  endemic  to  the  Netherlands,  is  reported  as  possibly 
extinct  (Wells  et  ai,  1983),  but  has  been  found  living  in 
France  (Platts,  1985).  Moreover,  D.  batava  may  represent 
an  introduction  of  a  previously  described  species  from 
elsewhere  in  the  world  (Wells  et  ai,  1983;  T.  Gosliner, 
pers.  comm.,  1990). 

Six  to  eight  species  of  brackish  water  hydrobiid  snails 
were  reported  as  possibly  extinct  on  the  United  States 
Atlantic  coast  by  Morrison  (1970).  The  distinction  of  these 
undescribed  species  from  still  living  and  closely  related 
taxa  has  not  been  demonstrated  (F.  Thompson,  pers. 
comm.,  1986),  nor  is  it  clear  that  searches  were  made  for 
still  extant  populations. 

The  most  intriguing  record  that  we  have  found  is  that 
of  the  Californian  potamidid  estuarine  snail  Cerithidea 
fuscata,  which  Taylor  (1981)  reported  as  "possibly  ex- 


78 


J.  T.  CARLTON  ET  AL. 


tinct."  This  high  intertidal,  mudflat-dwelling  horn  snail 
is  known  only  from  San  Diego  Bay  in  southern  California; 
it  was  last  collected  in  1935.  Taylor  ( 198 1 )  suggested  that 
threats  to  its  existence  were  "pollution,  dredging,  and  land 
fill."  Taylor  (1981)  treated  C.  fuscata  Gould,  1857,  as  a 
distinct  species,  with  Cerithidea  sacrata  hyporhyssa  Berry, 
1 906,  in  synonymy.  Grant  and  Gale  (1931)  and  Bequaert 
(1942)  considered  the  latter  a  synonym  of  Cerithidea  cal- 
ifornica  (Haldeman,  1840).  The  status  of  C.  fuscata  as  a 
species  distinct  from  Cerithidea  californica.  rather  than 
either  an  ecophenotype  or  subspecies,  has  not  been  clar- 
ified (J.  McLean,  pers.  comm.,  1986).  Cerithidea  fuscata 
differs  from  other  populations  of  Cerithidea  by  virtue  of 
its  smooth,  tapered  shell  with  flat  whorls  (Berry,  1906). 
While  C.  californica  is  common  and  widespread  both  to 
the  north  and  south,  the  smooth-shell  population  has  long 
been  considered  to  occur  only  in  San  Diego  Bay  (Burch, 
1945).  There  are  no  details  of  population  declines  or  dis- 
appearances of  C.  fuscata  as  yet  documented,  nor  is  there 
published  evidence  that  searches  have  been  made  for  ex- 
tant populations.  Nevertheless,  that  populations  of  Cer- 
ithidea. whose  life  history  is  characterized  by  non-plank- 
tonic  larvae  (Race,  1981),  are  susceptible  to  bay-wide  ex- 
tinctions has  been  documented  elsewhere  (Carlton,  1976). 
It  is  clear  from  these  and  other  reports  that  there  are 
historical  records  of  marine  and  estuarine  mollusks  with 
small  and  geographically  limited  populations,  and  that 
some  of  these  populations  are  believed  to  have  disap- 
peared. There  are  also  many  species  of  crustaceans,  an- 
nelids, flatworms,  hydroids,  and  other  invertebrates  that 
have  never  been  reported  since  the  nineteenth  century. 
Some  of  these  are  from  coastal  localities  that  have  been 
obliterated  during  the  course  of  human  population  ex- 
pansion and  concomitant  littoral  urbanization.  Finally, 
at  both  local  (state)  and  international  levels,  various  ma- 
rine invertebrates  have  been  reported  as  "endangered." 
Listings  for  marine  mollusks  have  been  achieved  in  part 
due  to  the  collecting  activities  of  shell  collectors.  We  dis- 
tinguish all  of  these  records  from  demonstrably  extinct 
taxa. 

Conclusions 

With  its  specialized  feeding  habits  and  narrow  habitat 
range,  Lottia  alveus  conforms  well  to  the  profile  of  species 
that  are  believed  to  be  highly  susceptible  to  extinction 
(Martin  and  Klein,  1984;  Vermeij,  1986).  The  limited 
geographic  range  (a  consequence  of  Pleistocene  glacia- 
tions),  the  limited  trophic  range  (an  adaptation  dating 
from  the  Mio-Pliocene),  and  the  presumably  limited 
physiological  range  (a  phylogenetic  constraint  shared  by 
almost  all  lottiid  limpets  and  perhaps  dating  from  the 
Paleozoic  origin  of  the  group)  were  interwoven  and  cas- 
cading attributes  that  set  the  stage  to  make  this  species 


vulnerable  to  extinction.  We  suggest  that  the  refugia  of 
Lotlia's  sole  food  source  during  a  period  of  catastrophic 
decline  were  outside  of  this  limpet's  habitat  range,  and 
the  limpet  became  extinct. 

Most  marine  invertebrates  whose  biology  and  distri- 
bution are  well-known  do  not  have  habits  as  specialized 
and  ranges  as  narrow  as  did  Lottia  alveus.  (There  are,  of 
course,  a  great  many  species  described  from  only  one  lo- 
cality, but  whose  actual  ranges  are  not  known).  Most  of 
those  taxa  that  are  known  from  one  or  a  few  host  species 
have  much  wider  geographical  ranges  than  did  this  limpet. 
The  fact  that  most  marine  invertebrates  have  large  effec- 
tive population  sizes,  often  over  broad  ranges,  may  ac- 
count further  for  their  relative  invulnerability  to  extinction 
in  historical  time.  In  contrast,  small  and  geographically 
restricted  populations  of  species  (short-range  endemics, 
for  example)  may  be  particularly  vulnerable  to  extinction. 
Those  species  whose  life  history  combines  non-planktonic 
larvae  with  juvenile  or  adult  stages  not  likely  to  be  asso- 
ciated with  drifting  algae  or  wood  may  be  specifically  sus- 
ceptible to  extinction. 

While  our  records  to  date  indicate  that  marine  inver- 
tebrates in  general  have  escaped  historical  extinctions  by 
the  end  of  the  twentieth  century,  human  activities  have 
been  and  are  clearly  capable  of  severely  reducing  and 
completely  eliminating  populations  of  marine  inverte- 
brates from  extensive  parts  of  their  ranges.  These  actions 
have  and  will  continue  to  fundamentally  alter  the  structure 
of  natural  communities. 

Acknowledgments 

We  thank  numerous  museum  curators  and  correspon- 
dents for  aid  in  establishing  the  status  of  this  small  snail. 
Frederick  Short  (University  of  New  Hampshire)  provided 
the  herbarium-based  record  from  Quebec.  Ralph  Dexter 
(Kent  State  University)  provided  valuable  information 
and  references  on  his  mollusk  collections  at  Cape  Ann, 
Massachusetts.  Terrence  Gosliner  (California  Academy 
of  Sciences).  James  McLean  (Los  Angeles  County  Mu- 
seum of  Natural  History),  and  Fred  Thompson  (Florida 
State  Museum,  Gainesville)  provided  valuable  comments 
on  the  taxonomic  status  of  certain  mollusks.  Peter  Frank 
(University  of  Oregon),  Peter  Petraitis  (University  of 
Pennsylvania),  and  Janie  Wulff  (Williams  College)  com- 
mented upon  various  versions  of  the  manuscript. 

Literature  Cited 

Abbott,  R.  T.  1954.  American  Seashells.  D.  Van  Nostrand  Company, 
Inc.,  Princeton.  NJ. 

Abbott,  R.  T.  1974.  .-lincnain  Seashells.  2nd  ed.  Van  Nostrand  Rein- 
hold  Company.  New  York  City,  NY. 

Bequaert,  J.  1942.  Random  notes  on  American  Potamididae.  Nautilus 
56:  20-30. 


EXTINCT  MARINE  INVERTEBRATE 


7') 


Berry,  S.  S.  1906.  Note  on  a  new  variety  of  Centhidea  sacrala  Gld., 

from  San  Diego,  Cal.  Nautilus  19:  133. 
Burch,  J.  Q.  1945.     Ccrilhidea  California]  hyporhyssa.  Mm   Concho- 

logical  Club  So.  Calif.  54:  34-35. 
Burch,  J.  Q.  1946.     Family  Acmaeidae.  Mm.  Conchological  Club  So 

Calif.  57:  5-16. 
Carlton,  J.  T.  1976.     Extinct  and  endangered  populations  ofthe  endemic 

mudsnail  Cerithidea  californica  in  northern  California.  Bull  Am 

Malacol  L'nioii  1975:  65. 
Clark,  K.  B.  1975.     Nudibranch  life  cycles  in  the  Northwest  Atlantic 

and  their  relationship  to  the  ecology  of  fouling  communities,  He/go/. 

Wiss.  Meercsuntcrs.  27:  28-69. 
Conrad,  T.  1831.     Descriptions  of  fifteen  new  species  of  Recent,  three 

of  fossil  shells,  chiefly  from  the  coast  ofthe  United  States.  J  Acad 

Nat.  Sci.  Philadelphia  6:  256-268. 
Couthouy,  J.  P.  1839.     Monograph  on  the  family  Osteodesmacea  of 

Deshayes,  with  remarks  on  two  species  of  Patelloidea.  and  descriptions 

of  new  species  of  marine  shells,  a  species  ofAnculotus,  and  one  of 

£0/1.5.  Boston  J.  Nat.  Hist.  2:  129-189. 
Dall,  VV.  H.  1871.     On  the  limpets;  with  special  reference  to  the  species 

ofthe  west  coast  of  America,  and  to  a  more  natural  classification  of 

the  group.  Am.  J  Conch.  6:  228-282. 
Dall,  VV.  H.  1914.     Notes  on  some  northwest  coast  acmaeas.  Nautilus 

28:  13-15. 
Dall,  VV .  H.  1921.     Summary  ofthe  marine  shell-hearing  mollusks  of 

the  northwest  coast  of  Alaska.  Bull.  U.  S  Nail.  Mils.  112:  1-217. 
De  Kay,  J.  E.  1843.     Zoology  of  New  York.  Part  \'  Mollusca.  Carroll 

and  Cook,  Albany. 
den  Hartog,  C.  1970.     The  Sea  Grasses  of  the  H'orld  North-Holland 

Publishing  Company.  Amsterdam. 
den  Hartog,  C.  1987.     "Wasting  disease"  and  other  dynamic  phenomena 

in  eelgrass  beds.  Ai/ual.  Bo/  27:  3-14. 
Dexter,  R.  \V .  1950.     Restoration  ofthe  Zostera  faciation  at  Cape  Ann, 

Massachusetts.  Ecology  31:  286-288. 
Dexter,  R.  W.  1962.     Further  studies  on  the  marine  mollusks  of  Cape 

Ann.  Massachusetts.  Nautilus  76:  63-70. 
Dexter,  R.  VV .  1968.     Distribution  ofthe  marine  molluscs  at  Cape  Ann. 

Massachusetts.  Proc.   Symp.   Mollusca  (Mar.   Biol.   Assoc.   India, 

Cochin.  Symp.  Ser.  3)  I:  214-222. 
Dreyer,  W.  A.,  and  W.  A.  Castle.  1941.     Occurrence  ofthe  bay  scallop, 

Pecten  irradians.  Ecology  22:  425-427. 

Durham,  J.  VV.,  and  F.  S.  MacNeil.  1967.     Cenozoic  migrations  of  ma- 
rine invertebrates  through  the  Bering  Strait  region.  Pp.  326-349  in 

The  Bering  Land  Bridge.  D.  M.  Hopkins,  ed.  Stanford  University 

Press,  Stanford,  CA. 
Emerson,  VV.  K.,  and  M.  K.  Jacobson.  1976.     The  American  Museum 

of  Natural  History  Guide  to  Shells.  Alfred  A.  Knopf,  New  York. 
Fishlyn,  D.  A.,  and  D.  VV.  Phillips.  1980.     Chemical  camouflaging  and 

behavioral  defenses  against  a  predatory  seastar  by  three  species  of 

gastropods  from  the  surfgrass  Phyllospadix  community.  Biol.  Bull 

158:  34-48. 
Franz,  D.  R.  1982.     An  historical  perspective  on  molluscs  in  lower  New 

York  harbor,  with  emphasis  on  oysters.  Pp.  181-197  in  Ecological 

Stress  and  the  New  York  Bight:  Science  and  Management,  G.  F. 

Mayer,  ed.  Estuarine  Research  Federation,  Columbia.  SC. 
Ganong,  W.  F.  1890.     Article  III.  Zoological  notes.  Report  ofthe  Com- 
mittee on  Marine  Invertebrate  Zoology.  I.  Mollusca.  Bull  Nat.  Hist. 

Soc.  New  Brunswick  9:  1-14. 

Gee,  J.  H.  R.,  and  K.  Wilson.  1981.     The  littoral  Mollusca  of  Green- 
island,  Co  Antrim:  40  years  of  change  in  Belfast  Lough.  Ir.  Nat.  J 

20:  343-345. 
Gibson-Smith,  J.,  and  W.  Gibson-Smith.  1981.     The  status  of  Pholu- 

domya  Candida  G.  B.  Sowerby.  I.  1823.  I'cliger  23:  355-356. 


Gould.  A.  A.,  and  VV .  G.  Binney.  1870.     Report  on  the  Invcrtchrata  nf 

Massachusetts.  Wright  and  Potter,  Boston,  MA. 
Grant,  U.  S.,  and  H.  R.  Gale.  1931.     Catalogue  ofthe  marine  Pliocene 

and  Pleistocene  Mollusca  of  California  and  adjacent  regions.  Mem. 

San  Diego  Soc.  Nat.  /list.  1:  1-1036. 

Jackson,  H.  1907.     The  differences  between  the  two  New  England  spe- 
cies of  Acmaea  Nautilus  21:  1-5,24. 
Jackson,  H.  1908.     The  Mollusca  of  North  Haven.  Maine.  Nautilus 

21:  142-144. 
Jensen,  K.,  and  K.  B.  Clark.  1983.  Annotated  checklist  of  Florida  as- 

coglossan  Opisthobranchia.  Nautilus  91:  1-13. 
Johnson,  C.  W.  1915.     Fauna  of  New  England  List  of  Mollusca.  Occ 

Pap.  Boston  Soc.  Nat  Hist.  13:  1-231. 
Johnson,  C.  W.  1928.     A  review  ofthe  New  England  limpets.  Nautilus 

41:  109-117. 

Johnson,  C.  W.  1929.     Acmaea  lesludinalis  (Mull.).  Nautilus  42:  103. 
Kotaka,  T.,  and  K.  Ogasawara.  1974.  A  new  abalone  from  the  Miocene 

of  Aomon  Prefecture,  northeast  Honshu,  Japan.  I  'enus33:  1 17-128. 
I  minimi.  N.  VV.  1909.  Shells  of  Maine.  A  catalogue  ofthe  land,  fresh- 
water and  marine  Mollusca  of  Maine.  Fourth  Ann.  Rep/  Stale  En- 
tomologist, Augusta,  ME.  Pp.  217-262. 
Lermond,  N.  VV.  1922.     One  haul  ofthe  dredge.  The  Maine  Naturalist. 

Journal  o/  the  Knox  Academy  of  Arts  and  Sciences  2:  1 38-140. 
Letson,  E.  1905.     Check  list  ofthe  Mollusca  of  New  York.  Bull  New 

York  S/a/e  Museum  88,  Zoology  I  I .  Pp.  1-112. 
Lindberg,  D.  R.  1981.     Acmaeidae  Gastropoda  Mollusca.  Invertebrates 

ofthe  San  Francisco  Bay  Estuary  System.  The  Boxwood  Press,  Pacific 

Grove,  CA. 
I  jndberg,  D.  R.  1984.     A  recent  specimen  otCollise/la  edmilche/li  from 

San  Pedro,  California  (Mollusca:  Acmaeidae).  Bull.  So.  Calif.  Acad. 

Sci.  S3:  148-151. 
Lindberg,  D.  R.  1986.     Name  changes  in  the  "Acmaeidae."  t'eliger  29: 

142-148. 
Lindberg,  D.  R.  1988.  The  Patellogastropoda.  Malacol.  Rev,  Suppl.  4: 

35-63. 
Linsley,  J.  H.  1845.  Catalogue  ofthe  shells  of  Connecticut.  Am.  J  Sci. 

48:271-286. 
Marcus,  E.  d.  B.-R.  1972.     Notes  on  some  opisthobranch  gastropods 

from  the  Chesapeake  Bay.  Ches.  Sci.  13:  300-317. 
Marsh,  G.  A.  1973.  The  Zostera  epifaunal  community  in  the  York  River, 

Virginia.  Ches.  Sci  14:  87-97. 
Martin,  P.  S.,  and  R.  G.  Klein,  eds.  1984.     Quarternary  Extinctions    \ 

Prehistoric  Revolution.  University  of  Arizona  Press,  Tucson,  AZ. 
McLean,  J.  H.  1966.     West  American  prosobranch  Gastropoda:  su- 

perfamilies  Patellacea,  Pleurotomariacea,  and  Fissurellacea.  Ph.D. 

dissertation,  Stanford  University,  Stanford,  CA. 
McNeely,  J.  A.,  K.  R.  Miller,  W.  V.  Reid,  R.  A.  Mittermeier,  and  T.  B. 

Werner.  1990.     Conserving  the  World's  Biological  Diversity.  Inter- 
national Union  for  Conservation  of  Nature  and  Natural  Resources, 

Gland,  Switzerland,  and  Washington,  D.  C. 
McRoy,  C.  P.  1968.     The  distribution  and  biogeography  of  Zostera 

manna  (eelgrass)  in  Alaska.  Pac.  Sci.  22:  507-513. 
Miner,  R.  W.  1 950.     Field  Book  of  Seashore  Life.  G.  P.  Putnam's  Sons, 

New  York  City.  NY. 
Morrison,  J.  P.  E.  1970.     Brackish  water  mollusks.  Malacologia  10: 

55-56. 
Morse,  E.  S.  1910.     An  early  stage  of  Acmaea.  Proc.  Bos.  Soc.  Natl. 

Hist.  34:  313-323. 
Morse,  E.  S.  1921.     Observations  on  the  Living  Gastempods  of  New 

England.  Peabody  Museum,  Salem,  Massachusetts. 
Moskalev,  L.  I.  1967.     Collisella  angusla,  n.  sp.  Pp.  18-19  in  A.  N. 

Golikov  and  O.  A.  Scarlato.  Molluscs  ofthe  Possiet  Bay  (the  Sea  of 

Japan)  and  their  ecology.  Trudy  Zool.  Insl.  Leningrad  42:  5-154  (in 

Russian). 


80 


J.  T.  CARLTON  ET  AL 


Muehlstein,  L.  K.,  D.  Porter,  and  K.  T.  Short.  1988.     Labyrinthula  sp., 

a  marine  slime  mold  producing  the  symptoms  of  wasting  disease  in 

eelgrass,  Zostera  marina.  Mar.  Bin/.  99:  465-472 
Nagle,  J.  S.  1968.     Distribution  of  the  epibiota  of  macroepibenthic 

plants.  Contrib.  Mai   Sci   13:  105-144. 
O'Connor,  J.  S.  1972.  The  benthic  macrofauna  of  Moriches  Bay,  New 

York.  Biol.  Bull  142:  84-102. 
Platts,  E.  1985.     An  annotated  list  of  the  North  Atlantic  Opisthobran- 

chia.  excluding  Thecosomata  and  Gymnosomata.  Ophelia  Supple- 
ment 2:  150-170. 
Proctor,  \V.  1933.     Biological  survey  of  the  Mount  Desert  region.  Part 

V.  Wistar  Institute  of  Anatomy  and  Biology.  Philadelphia,  PA. 
Race,  M.  S.  1981.     Field  ecology  and  natural  history  ofCerilliidea  cal- 

ifornica  (Gastropoda:  Prosobranchia)  in  San  Francisco  Bay.  I'eliger 

24:  18-27. 
Rasmussen,  E.  1973.  Systematics  and  ecology  of  the  Isefjord  marine 

fauna  (Denmark).  (With  a  survey  of  the  eelgrass  ( Zostera)  vegetation 

and  its  communities).  Ophelia  II:  1-495 
Rasmussen,  E.  1977.     The  wasting  disease  of  eelgrass  (Zoslera  marina) 

and  its  effects  on  environmental  factors  and  fauna.  Pp.  1-52  in  Sea- 
grass  Ecosystems:  A  Scientific  Perspectives.  C.  P.  McRoy  and  C. 

Helfferich.  eds.  Marcel  Dekker.  New  York. 
Rathbun,  R.  1881.     The  littoral  marine  fauna  of  Provincetown,  Cape 

Cod,  Massachusetts.  Proc.  U.  S  Nail.  Mils.  3:  1 16-133. 
Runnegar,  B.  1979.     Pholutlomya  Candida  Sowerby:  the  last  cadaver 

unearthed.  I  'eliger  22:  171-172. 
Russell-Hunter,  W.  D.,  and  J.  S.  Tashiro.  1985.     Life-habits  and  infaunal 

posture  ofCitmingia  telltnoiiles  (Tellinacea,  Semelidae):  an  example 

of  evolutionary  parallelism.  I'digcr  27:  253-260. 
Short,  F.  T.,  A.  C.  Mathieson,  and  J.  I.  Nelson.  1986.     Recurrence  of 

the  eelgrass  wasting  disease  at  the  border  of  New  Hampshire  and 

Maine,  USA.  Mar.  Ecol.  Prog.  Set:  29:  89-92. 


Short,  F.  T.,  L.  K.  Muehlstein,  and  D.  Porter.  1987.  Eelgrass  wasting 
disease:  cause  and  recurrence  of  a  marine  epidemic.  Biol.  Bull  173: 
557-562. 

Short,  F.  T.,  B.  \V.  Ibelings,  and  C.  den  Hartog.  1988.     Comparison  of 
a  current  eelgrass  disease  to  the  wasting  disease  of  the  1930s.  Auiuii 
But.  30:  295-304. 

Stauffer,  R.  C.  1937.  Changes  in  the  invertebrate  community  of  a  lagoon 
after  disappearance  of  the  eelgrass.  Ecology  18:  427-431. 

Stimpson,  \V.  1851.  Shells  of  New  England.  A  Revision  of  the  Synonymy 
nl  the  Testaceous  Mo/lusks  of  New  England.  Phillips.  Sampson,  and 
Co..  Boston. 

Sumner,  F.  B.,  R.  C.  Osburn,  and  L.  J.  Cole.  1913.  A  biological  survey 
of  the  waters  of  Woods  Hole  and  vicinity.  Section  III.  A  catalogue 
of  the  marine  fauna.  Bull.  Bur.  Fisheries  31:  545-794. 

Taylor,  D.  \V.  1981.  Freshwater  mollusks  of  California:  a  distributional 
checklist.  Calif  /•'/.*•/;  Game  67:  140-163. 

Thompson,  L.  D.  1921.  Collecting  at  Nahant  Beach,  Mass.  Nautilus 
34:  100- 10 L 

\  ermeij,  G.  J.  1986.  The  biology  of  human-caused  extinction.  Pp.  28- 
49  in  The  Preservation  of  Species.  B.  G.  Norton,  ed.  Princeton  Uni- 
versity Press,  Princeton,  NJ. 

Hells,  S.,  R.  Pyle,  and  N.  Collins.  1983.  The  IUCN  Invertebrate  Red 
Data  Book  International  Union  for  Conservation  of  Nature,  Swit- 
zerland. 

\\icksten,  M.  K.  1984.  Early  twentieth  century  records  of  marine  decapod 
crustaceans  from  Los  Angeles  and  Orange  counties,  California.  Bull. 
So.  Calif.  Acad.  Sci.  83:  12-42. 

Winkley,  H.  VV.  1909.     Essex  County  notes.  Nautilus  23:  86-87. 

Yokoyama,  M.  1926.  Fossil  shells  from  Sado.  J  Fac.  Sci.  Imp.  Univ. 
Tokyo.  Sec.  2.  1:249-312. 


Reference:  Bio/  Bull  180:  81-92.  (February.  1991) 


Passive  Suspension  Feeding  by  an  Octocoral 

in  Plankton  Patches:  Empirical  Test 

of  a  Mathematical  Model 

MARK  R.  PATTERSON 

Division  of  Environmental  Studies,  University  of  California,  Davis,  California  95676 


Abstract.  Feeding  rate  in  the  octocoral,  Alcyonium  sid- 
eriitin,  was  investigated  as  a  function  of  colony  size,  flow 
speed,  and  prey  concentration.  The  feeding  rate  decreases 
with  time  in  high  prey  concentrations.  A  model  of  passive 
suspension  feeding  is  formulated  that  successfully  predicts 
feeding  behavior.  At  low  prey  concentrations,  the  model 
predicts  a  linear  feeding  response  as  particle  flux  or  colony 
size  increases.  The  dominant  constraint  on  feeding  is  the 
"handling  time"  required  to  transfer  prey  from  tentacle 
to  pharynx  and  to  re-extend  the  tentacle.  The  time  con- 
stant of  prey  capture  shows  no  relation  to  particle  flux, 
in  agreement  with  the  model.  Another  constraint,  the 
"nitration  time,"  is  inversely  related  to  colony  size  and 
flow  speed.  Filtration  time  becomes  important  only  during 
feeding  in  sparse  prey  concentrations,  when  feeding  rate 
is  proportional  to  flow  speed,  colony  size,  and  prey  con- 
centration. In  the  field,  Alcyonium  colonies  reduce  nitra- 
tion time  by  orienting  at  right  angles  to  the  dominant 
flow  direction.  Feeding  efficiency  on  prey  patches  is  low 
and  inversely  related  to  flow  speed,  colony  size,  and  prey 
concentration.  Feeding  in  patches  is  not  a  simple  process 
for  this  octocoral,  because  colonies  will  "saturate"  with 
prey  before  all  polyps  have  successfully  captured  a  single 
prey  item. 

Introduction 

Suspension  feeding  occurs  in  nearly  all  animal  groups 
(J0rgensen,  1966),  and  virtually  every  body  of  water  pos- 
sesses a  guild  of  organisms  making  a  living  by  filtering 
the  soup  in  which  they  live.  Groups  that  have  received 
the  greatest  amount  of  attention  in  the  literature  are  active 
suspension  feeders,  i.e.,  those  organisms  that  generate  their 

Received  15  September  1989;  accepted  6  November  1990. 


own  feeding  currents.  Organisms  that  rely  exclusively  on 
environmentally  produced  currents  to  bring  them  food 
are  termed  passive  suspension  feeders. 

Experimental  studies  on  active  suspension  feeders  led 
to  the  formulation  of  the  first  mathematical  models  of 
suspension  feeding.  Decreases  in  the  concentration  of 
particles  in  closed  systems  containing  these  animals  could 
be  easily  monitored:  use  of  a  decreasing  exponential  model 
of  filtration  allowed  calculation  of  pumping  rate  (J0rgen- 
sen.  1943).  Coughlan  (1969)  reviews  the  use  of  the  ex- 
ponential model  in  calculating  pumping  rates  (sometimes 
erroneously  called  filtration  rates)  for  active  suspension 
feeders.  Filtration  efficiency  was  assumed  to  be  100%  in 
his  treatment.  Williams  (1982)  showed  that  if  this  as- 
sumption is  seriously  violated,  the  decline  in  cell  concen- 
tration will  be  a  double  exponential,  and  measured  de- 
clines cannot  be  easily  converted  into  a  filtration  or 
pumping  rate.  His  formulation  of  suspension  feeding  also 
predicts  that  the  apparent  filtration  rate  will  be  a  function 
of  time  as  physical  limitations  of  the  system  with  respect 
to  filtration  efficiency  become  important.  Thus,  apparent 
variations  in  filtration  rate  may  be  nothing  more  than 
manifestations  of  how  sieving  and  other  means  of  particle 
capture  (Rubenstein  and  Koehl,  1977)  interact  with  the 
population  of  cells  of  different  sizes  available  for  capture. 
Behavioral  modifications  of  pumping  rate  need  not  be 
invoked  to  explain  variation  in  pumping  rate.  Williams 
(1982)  provides  a  prescription  for  measuring  pumping 
rate  accurately  and  testing  for  any  behavioral  modifica- 
tions; this  involves  finding  a  particle  that  is  filtered  with 
100%  efficiency  by  the  organism  under  investigation.  Most 
active  suspension  feeders  such  as  bivalves  (Jorgensen, 
1975;  Mohlenberg  and  Riisgard,  1978;  Palmer  and  Wil- 
liams, 1980)  and  ascidians  (Fiala-Medioni,  1973,  1978a, 
b,  c,  d)  attain  remarkable  capture  efficiencies  for  the  small 


81 


82 


M    R    PATTERSON 


particles  on  which  they  feed  (bacterio-  and  phytoplank- 
ton).  Efficiencies  can  often  reach  100%  for  particles  on 
the  order  of  10  j*m  in  diameter,  and  thus  pumping  rates 
can  be  easily  measured  following  the  recommendations 
of  Williams  (1982). 

Predictions  of  mathematical  models 

Mathematical  models  have  also  been  used  to  clarify  the 
control  of  suspension  feeding.  Two  complementary  and 
not  entirely  separable  approaches  have  been:  ( 1 )  to  predict 
how  an  organism's  feeding  rate  should  relate  to  the  density 
or  quality  of  the  food  it  encounters  (Holling,  1965;  Emlen, 
1973;  Doyle,  1979;  Scale,  1982)  and,  (2)  to  see  whether 
suspension  feeding  organisms  maximize  the  rate  of  energy 
gain  (Lehman,  1976;  Lam  and  Frost,  1976). 

Both  Holling  (1965;  functional  response  type  I)  and 
Lehman  (1976)  predict  that  ingestion  or  filtering  rate 
should  show  a  linear  dependence  on  prey  availability  or 
density  up  to  some  saturation  value  in  organisms  such  as 
cnidarians,  where  encounter  rate  with  the  prey  is  deter- 
mined by  organism  size  and  environment  (in  this  case, 
flow  speed).  The  saturation  level  is  presumably  set  by  the 
digestive  physiology  of  the  organism,  e.g..  the  "packed 
gut"  assumption  of  Townsend  and  Hughes  (1981).  An 
implicit  assumption  is  that  all  prey  encountered,  or  at 
least  some  constant  fraction  of  them,  are  retained  by  the 

dN 

organism  (constant  efficiency);  symbolically,  -   -  =  K, 

dt 

where  N  =  number  of  prey  caught,  and  K  is  a  constant. 
K  can  be  further  decomposed:  K  =  U  X  V  X  SA,  where 
U  =  flow  speed,  V  =  prey  concentration,  and  SA  is  the 
surface  area  of  the  organism  available  for  prey  capture.  I 
term  this  hypothesis  the  "linear"  model  of  passive  sus- 
pension feeding,  which  is  typically  used  in  analyzing  pas- 
sive suspension  feeding. 

The  "linear"  model  predicts  that  for  a  given  prey  density 
below  the  saturation  level,  feeding  rate  should  be  constant. 
Figure  1 A  gives  the  solution  to  the  linear  model  and  shows 
how  doubling  the  prey  concentration,  flow  speed,  or  pro- 
jected surface  area  (size)  of  the  organism  should  affect  the 
feeding  "response."  Note  that  this  "filling"  curve  gives 
the  cumulative  number  of  prey  caught  as  a  function  of 
time;  it  assumes  that  prey  density  is  not  changing  as  the 
organisms  feeds.  The  "filling"  curve  is  mathematically 
isomorphic  with  the  functional  response  type  I  of  Holling 
(1965)  at  a  given  prey  concentration.  The  curve  is  also 
conceptually  equivalent  to  viewing  filtration  as  a  Poisson 
process,  i.e.,  the  probability  (P)  of  capture  during  a  small 
increment  of  time  (At)  is  constant,  and  the  magnitude  of 
P  is  the  product  of  U,  V,  and  SA  (Fig.  IB).  Furthermore, 
the  interval  between  capture  events  is  large  at  low  prey 
concentrations,  for  reasons  to  be  discussed  below,  and 
hence  capture  events  are  rare. 


saturation   level 


3 
O. 


2U 


B. 


Time 


Poisson  simulation  of  prey  capture 


500 
400 
300 
200 
100 
0 


P  =  06 


0  200  400  600  800 

Time  Units  (At) 

Figure  1.  The  classical  view  of  passive  suspension  feeding.  (A)  The 
"linear"  deterministic  model  of  passive  suspension  feeding,  which  assumes 
prey  encounter  rate  is  proportional  to  the  projected  surface  area  normal 
to  the  flow  (SA),  the  flow  speed  (U),  and  concentration  of  prey  present 
(V,).  The  feeding  rate  is  constant  until  some  saturating  level  of  prey 
inside  the  organism  is  attained.  This  curve  is  implicit  in  Holhng's(  1965) 
type  I  functional  response  curve  for  predator-prey  systems  similar  to 
passive  suspension  feeding.  (B)  The  previous  model  is  functionally 
equivalent  to  a  process  governed  by  the  Poisson  interval  distribution. 
i.e.,  a  process  where  the  probability  of  capture  (P)  during  a  small  interval 
of  time  (At)  is  constant.  The  filling  curves  were  generated  by  computer 
simulation  for  three  levels  of  P,  corresponding  to  increasing  levels  of 
flow,  colony  size,  or  particle  concentration.  Note  the  linear  dependence 
between  the  number  of  particles  captured  and  the  time  the  suspension 
feeder  has  been  exposed  to  a  current  carrying  prey  items. 


Energy  maximization  arguments  (cf.  Townsend  and 
Hughes,  1981)  argue  that  filter  feeders  should  feed  pref- 
erentially on  particles  with  the  higher  nutritional  value, 
unless  the  cost  of  sorting  and  rejection  are  too  high.  The 
few  tests  in  the  literature  (Doyle,  1979,  amphipod;  Scale, 
1982,  anuran  tadpole  larvae)  indicate  that  these  suspen- 
sion feeders  do  behave  in  a  manner  consistent  with  energy 
maximization.  Some  work  has  addressed  whether  models 
formulated  for  other  organisms  make  predictions  com- 
patible with  observations  of  feeding  rate  as  a  function  of 
prey  density  for  passive  suspension  feeding  in  cnidarians 
(Clayton  and  Lasker,  1982).  Sebens  (1979,  1984)  for- 
mulated a  cost/benefit  model  for  cnidarians  using  energy 
maximization  as  a  means  of  predicting  optimum  organism 
size  in  a  given  habitat.  The  model  has  had  good  success 


MODEL  OF  PASSIVE  SUSPENSION  FEEDING 


83 


in  predicting  maximum  organism  sizes  observed  in  the 
field.  However,  little  attention  has  been  paid  to  modeling 
the  response  of  passive  suspension  feeders  to  dense  prey 
concentrations,  which  may  change  suddenly  in  time,  i.e., 
what  happens  when  a  plankton  patch  sweeps  by  a  cni- 
darian  colony? 

A  dynamic  mathematical  model  of  passive 
suspension  feeding 

The  limitations  of  the  "linear"  model  are  those  imposed 
by  its  assumptions.  Feeding  rates  may  not  be  constant 
overtime,  especially  if  the  handling  of  individual  particles, 
or  digestive  or  neurally  mediated  behavior  becomes  im- 
portant. A  more  robust  model  of  suspension  feeding  for 
cnidarian  colonies  was  formulated  and  tested  against  real 
feeding  in  "patch"  concentrations  in  the  laboratory.  The 
model  takes  a  systems  analysis  view  of  passive  suspension 
feeding:  the  input  to  the  system  (colony)  is  prey  in  the 
water  column,  the  output  is  prey  inside  the  organism. 
The  model  allows  sudden  changes  in  prey  concentration 
and  predicts  the  time  course  of  feeding  using  two  param- 
eters. Congruence  between  the  observed  and  predicted 
parameters  of  the  model  implies  the  assumptions  used  in 
the  formulation  are  not  too  far  from  reality,  i.e.,  identi- 
fication has  been  made  of  the  salient  features  of  the  fil- 
tration system  that  determine  feeding  performance. 

There  are  three  model  assumptions.  ( 1 )  The  colony 
"fills"  up  with  prey  at  a  rate  proportional  to  the  difference 
between  the  ambient  plankton  concentration  (V,)  and  the 
amount  of  prey  already  in  the  colony  (V0);  symbolically, 
(V,  -  V0).  (2)  The  colony  fills  at  a  rate  inversely  propor- 
tional to  the  time  necessary  to  handle  the  particles  caught 
during  filtering  (R,)  and  the  time  needed  to  filter  the 
water  containing  the  particles  (R2);  symbolically. 

— —  .  Operationally,  R,  is  the  time  taken  to  transfer, 
(R,  +  R:) 

from  tentacle  to  polyp  mouth,  the  particle  caught  from  a 
unit  volume  of  water  and  to  re-extend  the  tentacle;  R2  is 
the  inverse  of  the  filtration  rate,  which  depends  on  the 
projected  area  of  the  organism  perpendicular  to  the  flow, 
and  the  flow  speed.  (3)  A  sudden  jump  in  the  plankton 
concentration  results  in  a  jump  in  the  number  of  particles 
caught.  The  size  of  the  jump  is  directly  proportional  to 

dV 

the  jump  in  the  particle  concentration, .the  colony 

dt 

volume  (C).  and  the  proportion  of  time  spent  filtering 

particles  during  feeding, -  ;  symbolically,  C  - 

(Ri  ~1~  R->)  dt 

R2 

(R,  +  R2) ' 

The  first  and  second  assumptions  address  the  steady- 
state  behavior  of  the  passive  suspension  feeder,  while  the 
third  deals  with  the  dynamic  aspect  of  prey  capture.  Ex- 


pressed  as  a  differential  equation,  passive  suspension 
feeding  may  obey: 


dV0  _   (V,  ~  Vo) 


, 


R 


.      0*1) 


dt        (R,  +  R2)          (R,  +  R2)  dt 

dV0 

where  C  -    -  is  the  time  change  in  the  total  number  of 
dt 

particles  caught  by  the  organism. 

Dividing  by  the  size  of  the  colony  yields: 


dV0        (V,  -  V0) 


dt       (R,  +  R:)C      (R,  +  R2)  dt 
Eq.  (2)  can  be  rearranged  algebraically  to: 


^.       (Eq.2) 


where  r  =  (R,  +  R:)  C.  and  «  = 


(Eq.3) 


is  the  time 


(R,  +  R2) 

constant  of  colony  "filling,"  while  a  is  the  measure  of 
how  many  prey  are  caught  as  the  edge  of  the  patch  sweeps 
by  the  colony. 

The  solution  to  Eq.  (3)  depends  on  the  nature  of  the 
change  in  the  plankton  concentration  in  the  water  column 
or  laboratory  flume.  An  electrical  circuit  that  mimics  ex- 
actly the  behavior  of  this  mathematical  feeding  model  is 
called  a  lag-lead  network  (Milsum,  1966)  and  is  shown 
in  Figure  2.  Formulation  of  the  resistive-capacitive  analog 
is  motivated  by  the  observation  that  prey  filtration  and 
prey  handling  are  discrete  processes.  They  are  modeled 
as  "resistances"  through  which  the  "current"  of  prey  must 
pass  to  fill  the  organism's  "capacity"  (the  etymological 
root  of  capacitance).  This  circuit  can  be  easily  wired  up 
with  variable  resistors,  Rj  2  and  variable  capacitor,  C,  al- 
lowing exploration  of  the  model's  qualitative  behavior.  A 
change  in  plankton  concentration  would  be  simulated  by 


o vw 


t    Rl  t 

I  CT     I 


V: 


I 


Figure  2.  Electrical  analog  to  the  differential  equations  used  to  model 
the  process  of  passive  suspension  feeding.  The  behavior  of  this  circuit 
exactly  mimics  the  model.  R,  is  the  handling  time  "resistance"  and  R2 
is  the  filtration  time  "resistance."  The  concentration  of  particles  in  the 
water  column  is  a  "voltage"  (V,)  that  may  cause  a  "current"  of  particles 
to  enter  and  reside  inside  the  organism  (V0);  this  is  controlled  by  the 
resistances,  R, :  and  the  volume  "capacitance"  (C)  of  the  animal. 


84 


M.  R.  PATTERSON 


a  change  in  the  input  voltage  (V,);  simulation  of  colony 
feeding  response  predicted  by  the  model  can  be  seen  by 
watching  the  behavior  of  V0  as  a  function  of  time  on  an 
oscilloscope. 

Applying  KirchofFs  law  to  this  circuit.  I  obtain: 


iR,  + 


E/« 


:  =  Vj(t) 


and 


c/' 


idt  +  iR:  =  V0(t), 


(Eq.4) 


(Eq.5) 


where  i  is  the  "current"  of  particles. 

Taking  the  Laplace  transform  of  Eqs.  (4)  and  (5)  yields: 


R,  +  R:  + 


=  V,(s)  (Eq.6) 


and 


R:  +  —  li(s)  =  V0(s),  (Eq.7) 


where  s  is  the  frequency-domain  variable.  Some  algebra 
then  results  in: 


V0(s) 
V,(s) 


RI  +  R2 


a(s  +  b) 
b(s  +  a) 


(Eq.8) 


Cs 


1 


-,  and  b  = 


1 


in       i     p    \  /-  p    /-  ' 

( ix  i   T~  K-2/  *-  rS-2*- 

Eq.  (8)  is  the  Laplace  transform  of  Eq.  (3).  It  can  be 
rearranged  to: 

V,(s) 


V0(s)  =  s 


Vj(s)  + 


(Eq.9) 


(rs  +  1)  (S  +   1) 

To  solve  Eq.  (9),  the  nature  of  the  input  change  in 
plankton  concentration  must  by  specified.  For  a  step 
increase  in  the  plankton  availability  to  level  V,,  caused 
by  a  patch  of  plankton  flowing  past  the  colony,  V,(s) 

V, 
-  .  Substituting,  I  obtain: 


Vi 

s 


V0(s)  =  s  - 

(rs  -r  i 

which  can  be  rearranged  to: 
R2CV, 


V,(s) 
(TS+  1) 


(Eq. 10) 


u,-,       -,  .  -•          (Eq.  11) 

(rs  +  1)       s(rs  +  1) 

Taking  the  inverse  Laplace  transform,  I  obtain: 

V0(t)  =  V,j  1  -  (1  -  «)e(-t/r)},          (Eq.  12) 
the  solution  in  the  time  domain. 


Since  «  and  T  can  be  computed  from  known  quantities, 
it  is  possible  to  compare  predicted  with  observed  values 
of  these  two  model  parameters.  In  particular,  T  will  be  an 
important  descriptor  of  how  quickly  a  colony  can  use  a 
change  in  plankton  concentration.  Figure  3A  shows  the 
"filling"  curve  for  colonies  of  different  size  (C),  while  Fig- 
ure 3B  shows  identically  sized  colonies  as  the  ratio  between 
"handling"  time  (R,)  and  "nitration"  time  (R:)  changes. 
The  implications  of  the  behavior  of  this  model,  its  de- 
composition into  the  "linear"  model  (Type  I  functional 
response)  under  certain  conditions,  and  the  extent  of  its 
congruence  with  reality  will  be  more  fully  developed  in 
the  Discussion  section. 

I  experimentally  tested  this  model  by  measuring  feeding 
rates  for  a  colonial  cnidarian  in  the  laboratory.  Alcyonium 
siderium,  an  octocoral,  is  a  dominant  zooplanktivore  on 
subtidal  hard  rock  substrates  in  New  England  (Sebens  and 
Koehl,  1984;  Sebens,  1986).  Colonies  assume  a  variety  of 
shapes  varying  from  fingers  to  globose  forms  to  com- 
pressed ellipsoids  (Patterson,  1980).  In  plankton-rich 


A. 


B. 


200         400        600         800 
Time  Units  (At) 


Equal  C 


a      0.8   ' 


O. 
•o 

-I 
a 

i- 
o 


200         400        600         800 
Time  Units  (At) 


1000 


Figure  3.  The  time  course  of  organism  feeding  as  predicted  by  the 
model.  (A)  Colonies  differing  in  size  by  a  factor  of  five  (C.  5C)  and  with 
handling  time  ( R  i )  much  greater  than  filtration  time  (R2),  as  occurs  during 
feeding  in  plankton  patches.  (B)  Colonies  of  identical  size  where  the 
handling  time  (R,)  is  much  greater  than  filtration  time  (R2)  and  vice 
versa.  The  ordinates  for  both  graphs  are  normalized  for  the  effects  of 
colony  size.  Note  that  if  R:  becomes  much  larger  than  R,,  i.e..  the  particle 
flux  drops,  then  the  filling  curve  tends  toward  a  step  function.  Since 
particle  flux  is  low,  each  passing  particle  is  caught,  and  the  model  de- 
composes to  the  "linear"  model  of  Figure  1. 


MODEL  OF  PASSIVE  SUSPENSION  FEEDING 


85 


habitats,  fully  expanded  globose  colonies  can  reach  10cm 
in  diameter.  Previous  work  with  Alcyonium  colonies 
feeding  in  a  closed  system  has  shown  that  they  readily 
accept  prey  particles  (Patterson,  1984).  This  octocoral 
might  be  expected  to  follow  the  "linear"  filling  curve  if 
prey  concentration  is  held  constant,  and  obey  the  changes 
predicted  in  Figure  1  as  size  and  flow  speed  are  varied. 
The  aim  of  this  laboratory  feeding  study  was  to  test  the 
"linear"  model  and  the  proposed  alternative  model  for 
feeding  in  plankton  patches.  An  intensive  study  of  the 
diet  of  this  species  over  a  diel  cycle  (Sebens  and  Koehl, 
1984)  provides  information  useful  in  analyzing  the  results 
of  this  study. 

Materials  and  Methods 

Colony  collection,  maintenance,  and  flow  generation 
and  measurement 

Feeding  rate  experiments  were  conducted  at  the  Marine 
Science  Center  (MSC),  Northeastern  University,  Nahant, 
Massachusetts,  and  in  the  biomechanics  laboratory  at  the 
University  of  California,  Davis.  Colonies  of  Alcyonium 
siderium  were  collected  by  SCUBA  diving  and  maintained 
in  flowing  seawater  tables  or  recirculating  chilled  aquaria. 
Feeding  observations  were  made  in  a  recirculating  flume 
described  in  Patterson  (1984).  All  experiments  were  per- 
formed with  the  flow  straighteners  installed,  which  re- 
moved turbulence  of  length  scales  greater  than  1  cm.  Flow 
speeds  and  turbulence  intensities  were  measured  with  a 
two  channel  thermistor  flowmeter  circuit  modified  from 
LaBarbera  and  Vogel  (1976).  The  voltage  output  of  the 
flowmeter  was  either  connected  to  an  eight-bit  successive 
approximation  A/D  converter  (Mountain  Computer) 
connected  to  an  Apple  He,  or  to  a  MacADIOS  A/D  con- 
verter (GW  Instruments)  connected  to  an  Apple  Macin- 
tosh Plus.  The  sampling  rate  was  10  Hz. 

Octocoral  colonies  attached  to  horse  mussels  (Modiolus 
modiolus)  were  collected  subtidally  from  1 5-23  m  depth. 
Mussel  shell  fragments  bearing  Alcyonium  colonies  were 
mounted  in  the  flow  tank  working  section.  The  prey  of- 
fered to  the  colonies  were  cysts  of  the  brine  shrimp,  Ar- 
temia  salina.  Characteristics  of  the  cysts  are  described  in 
Patterson  (1984).  Capture  of  the  cysts  on  individual  ten- 
tacles of  this  species  is  readily  observed.  At  the  end  of 
each  feeding  bout,  three  60-ml  samples  were  withdrawn 
isokinetically  using  a  Cole-Parmer  peristaltic  pump 
(model  no.  7568)  smoothed  with  hydraulic  capacitors. 
Samples  were  filtered  onto  gridded  Millipore  filters,  the 
number  of  cysts  was  counted,  and  a  mean  concentration 
of  particles  present  in  the  flow  was  calculated.  The  con- 
centration of  cysts  offered  (0.056-0.40  part./ml)  was  of 
the  order  of  plankton  concentrations  seen  in  the  field  (Se- 
bens and  Koehl,  1984).  However,  even  greater  concen- 
trations may  be  typical  of  dense  patches  of  plankton  that 


are  seasonally  and  spatially  abundant  (Fasham,   1978; 
Grosberg,  1982). 

Documenting  the  time  course  ofprcv  capture 

Alcyonium  colonies  were  introduced  individually  into 
the  working  section  of  the  flume  and  allowed  to  acclimate 
to  the  flow.  Prey  were  not  introduced  until  the  polyps 
were  fully  expanded.  A  standard  volume  concentration 
(0.45  g  dry  cysts/1)  of  Anemia  cysts  was  added  all  at  once 
to  the  flume.  Observations  of  capture  events  were  made 
at  a  magnification  of  35X  through  a  dissecting  microscope 
suspended  over  the  flume.  A  watch  glass  floating  on  the 
water  and  anchored  over  the  colony  prevented  blurring 
ot  the  image  from  capillary  waves  at  the  air/water  inter- 
face. An  interval  timer  program  (0.05  s  resolution)  running 
on  an  Apple  He  microcomputer  measured  the  time  be- 
tween capture  events.  The  time  required  for  a  tentacle  to 
transfer  a  captured  particle  to  the  pharynx  (JR,  [  in  the 
above  model)  was  timed  with  a  stop  watch  during  separate 
experiments. 

Filtration  time  for  an  individual  particle  was  calculated 
using  the  projected  surface  area  of  the  organism,  and  the 
flow  speed  measured  4  cm  upstream  of  the  top  of  the 
colony.  Specimen  volume  was  measured  by  volumetric 
displacement  of  water  in  a  graduated  cylinder.  The  num- 
ber of  prey  caught  as  a  function  of  time  was  plotted  for 
each  specimen;  the  observed  values  for  the  model  param- 
eters T-  and  a  were  obtained  using  a  least  squares  algorithm, 
and  then  compared  with  the  values  predicted  by  the  model 
calculations  through  linear  regression. 

Feeding  efficiency 

Efficiency  of  prey  capture  at  the  colony  level  was  com- 
puted as  follows:  the  number  of  particles  caught  by  a  col- 
ony during  a  standard  feeding  bout  of  10  min  was  divided 
by  the  number  of  particles  that  would  pass  through  the 
cross-sectional  area  occupied  by  the  colony  if  the  colony 
were  not  there.  This  is  the  standard  engineering  definition 
of  efficiency  of  particle  capture  (Dorman,  1966).  Because 
feeding  rate  at  dense  concentrations  of  prey  is  non-linear 
(Fig.  4),  efficiency  will  be  a  function  of  time.  Hence,  for 
purposes  of  comparison,  efficiency  is  computed  over  the 
time  necessary  to  reach  "saturation."  Saturation  is  defined 
as  the  point  at  which  capture  events  drop  to  less  than  one 
prey  item  caught  per  5  min  period  per  colony. 

Field  measurements  of  flow  and  orientation  to  flow 

Field  observations  of  orientation  to  flow  m  Alcyonium 
colonies  and  flow  regime  were  made  at  the  following  sites 
(depths)  in  the  subtidal  of  Massachusetts  Bay:  ( 1 )  Dive 
Beach  site  (8m),  located  near  Nahant,  Massachusetts 
(42°25'N:  70°54'W),  (2)  Shag  Rocks  inner  wall  (7  m) 


86 


M.   R    PATTERSON 


Step  response  of  Alcyonium  siderium 


Figure  4.  Typical  feeding  response  of  an  Alcyonium  colony  to  a  step 
increase  in  the  plankton  concentration  (Anemia  cysts).  The  time  axis 
(abscissa)  is  expressed  in  units  of  T,  a  model  parameter  denned  as  the 
time  necessary  for  the  cumulative  prey  capture  to  reach  (I  -  e~')  =  63% 
ol  the  saturating  value.  The  ordinate.  V.,  is  the  cumulative  number  of 
prey  captured  normalized  to  the  saturation  level.  Note  that  the  response 
is  curvilinear  and  can  be  characterized  by  two  parameters,  the  time  con- 
stant, 7,  and  a,  the  initial  jump  in  plankton  caught  as  the  concentration 
changes. 


located  near  Dive  Beach,  (3)  Shag  Rocks  outer  wall  (9 
m),  and  (4)  Halfway  Rock  (14m)  (42°30'N:  70°46'W). 
Orientation  to  the  direction  of  current  flow  by  colonies 
on  subtidal  rock  walls  was  measured  with  a  protractor 
and  plumb  line.  The  direction  of  current  flow  was  deter- 
mined with  a  filament  of  dye,  and  was  parallel  to  the 
bottom  and  the  wall.  Flow  measurements  were  made  in 
situ  at  1.0  cm  and  10.0  cm  height  over  Alcyonium  colonies 
using  a  submersible  thermistor  flowmeter  recording  a  dig- 
ital signal  on  magnetic  tape.  Flow  measurements  were 
made  over  a  three  year  period  in  all  kinds  of  weather 
throughout  the  year.  The  sampling  rate  was  3  Hz. 

Results 

Feeding  response  to  plankton  patch  concentrations 

When  Alcyonium  colonies  were  subjected  to  sharp  (step) 
increases  in  the  plankton  concentration,  the  "filling"  curve 
was  markedly  curvilinear  and  showed  an  asymptote  (see 
Fig.  4  for  a  typical  example).  Similar  results  were  obtained 
with  the  sea  anemone  Metridium  .v<w/t'(unpub.  data).  At 
these  high  prey  concentrations,  doubling  the  flow  speed 
and  hence  the  particle  flux  typically  had  little  effect  on 
the  feeding  curve  for  a  given  colony  (Fig.  5),  providing 
evidence  that  the  linear  model  of  passive  suspension  feed- 
ing doesn't  apply  very  well  in  patch  concentrations. 

Figure  4  gives  the  graphical  interpretation  of  T  and  a. 
Figure  6  shows  how  closely  the  model  formulated  in  the 
Introduction  predicts  T,  the  time  constant,  [time  needed 
to  reach  (l-e~')  of  saturation],  and  a,  the  proportion  of 
prey  caught  as  the  edge  of  the  patch  sweeps  past  the  colony 
at  the  start  of  feeding  bout.  Model  I  linear  regression  was 
used  to  test  the  ability  of  the  predicted  (calculated)  model 
parameters  to  forecast  the  observed  values.  This  type  of 


100   150   200   250   300   350   400   450 
Time  (sec) 


Figure  5.  Feeding  response  of  the  same  colony  of  Alcyonium  to  Ar- 
lemia  cysts  offered  at  very  different  flux  rates  (flow  speed  j  LI  |  X  particle 
concentration  |V,  ]).  This  effect  is  not  predicted  by  the  linear  model 
(Fig.  1 ). 


regression  analysis  is  appropriate  since  the  x  values  (the 
computed  model  parameters)  were  known  precisely  and 
fixed  by  the  choice  of  colony  (Sokal  and  Rohlf,  1981). 
Alpha  values  were  log  transformed  before  calculations  of 
the  regression  to  eliminate  problems  with  non-normality. 
The  aim  of  the  model  was  to  predict  feeding  behavior  in 
dense  suspensions  to  within  a  factor  of  two.  The  model 
achieves  this  goal  in  predicting  T  and  a.  Linear  regressions 
are  robs  =  6.9+1 .04  In  (rpred)  and  In  «obs  =  - 1 .93  +  0.28 
In  («pred).  R2  values  for  these  regressions  for  «  and  T  are 
0.33  (P  =  0.05)  and  0.70  (P  =  0.0006),  respectively. 


A. 


t  -  lime  constant  of  colony  feeding 


4UO 

A 

300 

'               *A 

200 

*fr 

A 

100 

A 

0               100            200            300            40 

Predicted  Value  (s) 

B. 


a  -  initial  patch  capture  fraction 


1.2 
1.0 
0.8 
0.6 
0.4 
0.2 


0.0       0.2       0.4       0.6       0.8        1.0        1.2 
Predicted  Value  (X  10''%) 

Figure  6.  Predicted  and  observed  values  of  (A)  the  time  constant,  T, 
and  ( B)  the  patch  edge  capture  traction.  «.  for  particle  nitration  by  colonies 
ofAlcyanium  siderium.  Predicted  values  of  the  model  parameters  were 
computed  from  the  handling  time  (R,),  nitration  time  (R2),  and  the 
colony  volume  (C). 


MODEL  OF  PASSIVE  SUSPENSION  FEEDING 


87 


300 


•=      200 


0 


01234     5     6789     10    11 
Particle  Flux  (particles/Jem  2    •  s}) 

Figure  7.  Plol  of  the  time  constant  of  colony  tilling,  T,  as  a  function 
of  the  particle  flux  (flow  speed  JUJ  X  particle  concentration  ]V,[).  The 
data  do  not  have  a  slope  significantly  different  from  zero  (P  <  0.001) 
showing  the  lack  of  dependence  of?-  on  particle  flux  at  high  flux  rates. 


Because  R,  >  R2  at  the  prey  concentrations  used,  and 
T  =  (R,  +  R:)  C,  R:  will  have  little  effect  on  T.  Thus  a 
corollary  to  the  model  is  that  particle  flux  past  the  colony 
for  high  prey  densities  will  have  no  correlation  with  the 
time  constant  (T)  or  organism  filling.  This  indeed  was  the 
case  (Fig.  7).  The  model  has  slightly  lower  success  in  pre- 
dicting the  magnitude  of  «,  which  measures  the  degree  to 
which  a  colony  can  "grab"  the  edge  of  a  plankton  patch 
as  it  sweeps  by.  Alpha  is  consistently  overestimated;  it  is 
probably  sensitive  to  colony  shape  and  the  precise  patterns 
of  flow  obtained  for  a  particular  shape,  and  these  aspects 
of  passive  suspension  feeding  were  not  part  of  the  model 
formulation. 

Colony  size  and  feeding  efficiency 

Figure  9  demonstrates  an  inverse  relationship  between 
efficiency  of  capture  (as  defined  in  the  Materials  and 
Methods)  and  colony  size.  Smaller  colonies  are  more  ef- 
ficient filters,  although  all  sizes  have  very  low  efficiencies 
when  feeding  in  dense  concentrations.  Figure  10  shows 
that  there  is  also  an  inverse  relationship  between  efficiency 
and  flow  speed,  and  hence  particle  flux,  for  a  given  particle 
concentration,  for  feeding  by  colonies. 


40 


20- 


mean  =  105  = 
N  =  210 


0      20     40     60     80    100  120  140    160  180 
Colony  long-axis  orientation  (°) 

Figure  8.  Orientation  of  the  longest  dimension  ofAlcyonium  colonies 
to  the  local  direction  of  current  flow  at  four  subtidal  sites  in  Massachusetts 
Bay.  Angles  were  measured  with  protractor  and  plumb  line;  current  di- 
rection was  determined  with  a  filament  of  sodium  fluorescein  dye. 


0       5       10     15     20     25     30     35     40     45 


Colony  Size,  S  (cnr*  ) 

Figure  9.  Efficiency  of  particle  capture  per  colony  (E)  in  Alcyonium 
as  a  function  of  colony  size  (S).  Efficiency  is  defined  as  the  number  of 
particles  caught  by  the  colony  in  the  time  interval  to  saturation  of  the 
colony  divided  by  the  number  of  particles  that  would  have  passed  through 
the  space  occupied  by  the  colony.  The  regression  is  given  by  the  equation: 
E  =  12.8  -  0.68S  +  (9.62  x  \0~3)S2(P  <  0.05;  R2  =  0.55;  df  =  II). 


Discussion 

The  dynamics  of  cnidarian  passive  suspension  feeding 

Most  cnidarians  use  passive  suspension  feeding,  even 
though  many  forms  such  as  scleractinian  corals  also  pos- 
sess symbiotic  dinoflagellates  that  supply  them  with  some 
large  fraction  of  their  nutrition  (Muscatine  and  Porter, 
1977).  While  the  independence  of  zooplankton  capture 
from  autotrophy  has  been  questioned  (Clayton  and  Las- 
ker,  1982),  there  is  no  doubt  that  for  most  boreal  cnidar- 
ians lacking  zooxanthellae,  capture  of  paniculate  prey 
from  the  water  column  is  of  prime  importance  in  their 
biology.  Hence,  modeling  of  the  passive  suspension  feed- 
ing process  is  worthwhile  because  (1)  it  is  ubiquitous  in 
marine  systems,  (2)  the  particles  filtered  from  the  water 
column  are  patchy  (Wiebe,  1970,  1971;  Ortner  et  a/., 
1984),  i.e..  discontinuously  distributed  in  space  and  time. 


S  10 
x 


£    4 

Ed 

.     2 


"S    o- 

£      o 


5         10       15       20       25       30       35 
Flow  Speed,  U  (cm/s) 


Figure  10.  Efficiency  (E)  of  particle  capture  per  colony  in  Alcyonium 
as  a  function  of  flow  speed  (U).  Efficiency  is  defined  as  the  number  of 
particles  caught  by  the  colony  in  the  time  interval  to  saturation  of  the 
colony  divided  by  the  number  of  particles  that  would  have  passed  through 
the  space  occupied  by  the  colony.  The  regression  is  given  by  the  equation: 
E  =  32.30U-|"(/)  <  0.05;  R:  =  0.81;  df  =  11).  These  flow  speeds  cor- 
respond to  a  Reynolds  number  (Re)  range  of  800-12.000  calculated 
using  the  greatest  dimension  of  each  colony. 


88  M.  R.  PATTERSON 

Table  I 

/•'/cM  measurement  <>l  tl<>»  v/xr</  /  II  cm  and  In  II  cm  above  colonies  <>/  Alcyonium  al  lour  \uhiulal  Mies  in  Massachusetts  Bay 
(December  1981-Sepiemher  1984) 


Site 

Depth  (m) 

1  .0  cm 

10.0cm 

Dives 

Flow  speed  (cm/s) 

Re  x  102 

Dives 

Row  speed  (cm/s) 

Re  x  102 

Halfway  Rock 
Shag  Rocks  Inner 
Shag  Rocks  Outer 
Dive  Beach 

14 
7 
9 
8 

10 
18 
20 
23 

18.8(9.8) 
8.7(7.2) 
10.5(8.5) 
9.3(7.1) 

90.8  (47.3) 
42.0  (34.8) 
50.7(41.11 
44.9(34.3) 

11 
18 
21 
24 

18.4(10.6) 
10.2(9.7) 
11.3(8.2) 
9.3(7.6) 

88.9(51.2) 
49.3  (46.9) 
54.6  (39.6) 
44.9  (36.7) 

Flow  was  sampled  at  3  Hz  for  6  minutes  per  date. 

Reynolds  number  (Re)  was  calculated  for  a  colony  5  cm  in  greatest  dimension. 

Values  are  mean  (standard  deviation). 


and  the  feeding  response  of  these  organisms  to  patches 
may  have  important  effects  on  growth  and  metabolism 
(Szmant-Froelich  and  Pilson,  1984),  or  competition  for 
food  and  space  (Okamura.  1984),  and  (3)  any  predictive 
model  must  make  assumptions  that  will  offer  insight  into 
which  features  of  the  system  are  the  most  important. 

Patchiness  of  particles  is  a  common  phenomenon  in 
aquatic  systems;  the  causes  can  be  both  biological  (nutrient 
tracking,  mass  spawning)  and  abiotic  (eddy  entrainment) 
in  origin  (reviewed  in  Okubo.  1980).  Passive  suspension 
feeders  respond  to  patchiness  in  interesting  ways.  Leversee 
(1976)  found  that  an  octocoral  could  alter  its  feeding  rate 
in  response  to  jumps  in  prey  concentration.  Crowell(  1957) 
observed  that  a  hydroid  grew  better  on  a  single  large  daily 
ration  of  food  than  more  frequent  feedings,  implicating 
more  efficient  digestion  in  a  packed  gut  (coelenteron). 
Lasker  el  al.  (1982)  discovered  that  Hydra  interrupts  its 
feeding  to  digest  its  prey;  therefore  a  single  large  feeding 
(dense  prey)  is  just  as  good  as  a  continuous  supply.  The 
response  of  Alcyoniitm  to  patch  concentrations  is  inter- 
esting, especially  when  contrasted  with  the  capture  of  prey 
in  the  field  under  more  dilute  conditions  (Sebens  and 
Koehl,  1984). 

Lasker  el  al.  ( 1982)  used  prey  concentrations  more  than 
an  order  of  magnitude  greater  than  those  used  in  this 
study.  Plankton  densities  over  coral  reefs  (Alldredge  and 
King,  1977)  and  near  boreal  subtidal  rock  walls  (K.  Se- 
bens, Northeastern  University,  pers.  comm.)  can  often  be 
greater  than  those  in  the  water  column  nearby.  Densities 
of  plankton  in  freshwater  aquatic  systems  are  typically 
on  the  order  of  tens  of  plankters  per  liter  (Wetzel,  1975); 
comparable  densities  are  found  in  New  England  inshore 
waters  (Sebens,  1984).  The  concentrations  used  in  this 
study  were  on  the  order  of  100/1.  By  comparison,  oligo- 
trophic  oceanic  waters  usually  have  less  than  one  zoo- 
plankter  per  liter  (Ortner,  et  al..  1981).  Feeding  on  high 
densities  of  prey  resulted  in  a  curvilinear  feeding  response 
for  the  octocoral  species  investigated  in  this  study.  Al- 


cyonium did  not  feed  markedly  faster  at  enhanced  levels 
of  prey  density,  in  contrast  to  previous  work  with  scler- 
actinians  (Lasker,  1976;  Clayton  and  Lasker,  1982). 

Handling  time,  filtration  time,  and  flow  in  the  field 

Part  of  the  error  in  the  predictions  of  the  parameter 
values  of  the  model  (Fig.  6)  can  be  attributed  to  the  vari- 
ation in  handling  time  (R,).  The  mean  handling  time  for 
a  particle  was  8.0  s  (SD  =  3.0;  n  =  20  colonies).  It  was 
not  possible  to  measure  handling  time  and  capture  events 
simultaneously,  so  the  mean  handling  time  was  used  to 
calculate  the  model  parameters.  The  filtration  resistance 
(R;),  is  not  subject  to  as  wide  a  variation  within  a  colony 
unless  the  organism  changes  its  size  by  pumping  water 
into  its  gastrovascular  spaces.  Size  change  is  usually  a  re- 
sponse to  severe  hydromechanical  stress  (cf.  Patterson, 
1980);  these  organisms  do  not  appear  to  regulate  feeding 
rate  through  size  changes  except  to  turn  feeding  on  and 
oft".  Robbins  and  Shick  (1980)  found  similar  behavior  in 
the  sea  anemone  Metridium  senile.  Colonies  did  not 
change  size  during  the  course  of  these  feeding  experiments. 
During  ontogeny,  the  potential  exists  for  Alcyoniuni  col- 
onies to  reduce  the  value  of  the  filtration  time,  R2,  by 
growing  in  an  oriented  fashion  to  the  predominant  direc- 
tion of  flow.  This  indeed  seems  the  case  (Fig.  8). 

The  flow  regime  experienced  by  these  colonies  over  a 
three  year  period  at  the  four  sites  demonstrates  a  wide 
range  of  flow  speeds  (Table  I),  with  mean  flows  on  the 
order  of  10-20  cm/s.  This  corresponds  to  a  whole  colony 
Reynolds  number  of  ca.  5,000  to  10,000.  The  flow  at  these 
depths  is  tidally  driven  and  is  often  dominated  by  wave- 
induced  oscillations  (Patterson  and  Sebens,  1989).  Col- 
onies generally  do  not  feed  as  velocities  approach  50  cm/ 
s  (unpubl.  obs.)  and  instead  begin  contraction. 

The  time  constant  of  organism  "filling"  (T)  depends  on 
colony  size,  handling  time,  and  filtration  time.  Handling 
time  was  so  much  larger  than  filtration  time  for  the  particle 


MODEL  OF  PASSIVE  SUSPENSION  FEEDING 


89 


fluxes  tested,  that  it  dominated  the  time  constant.  For 
example,  at  the  lowest  particle  flux  tested  |0.25  part./ 
(cm2  •  s)}.  the  handling  time  was  three  orders  of  magnitude 
greater  than  the  nitration  time  for  a  typical  colony  with 
a  projected  surface  area  of  10  cm2.  At  what  particle  flux 
would  the  handling  time  and  the  nitration  time  become 
comparable  in  magnitude,  i.e.,  at  what  particle  concen- 
tration and  flow  speed  would  passive  suspension  feeding 
be  expected  to  be  responsive  to  the  changes  in  particle 
flux?  For  the  same  size  colony  considered  above,  the  han- 
dling time  will  equal  the  nitration  time  at  a  particle  flux 
of  0.01  part./(cm2  •  s).  What  are  particle  fluxes  like  in  the 
field? 

Comparison  with  field  data:  do  colonies  become  more 
efficient  at  lower  particle  fluxes? 

Using  the  data  of  Sebens  (1984)  and  Sebens  and  Koehl 
(1984),  it  is  possible  to  calculate  how  many  particles  are 
caught  by  Alcyonium  in  the  field,  and  make  some  order 
of  magnitude  calculations  of  the  particle  flux  they  are 
experiencing.  Knowing  the  prey  caught  and  the  particle 
flux,  we  can  calculate  efficiency  of  capture.  Particle  flux 
is  the  product  of  prey  concentration  and  flow  speed.  Flow 
speeds  have  been  measured  (Sebens,  1984;  Table  I)  and 
Sebens  (1984)  reports  plankton  concentrations  averaging 
about  3500  zooplankters/m\  or  3.5  particles/1,  in  the 
warmer  months  of  the  year  at  the  Nahant,  Massachusetts, 
sites.  Flow  speeds  are  on  the  order  of  about  10-20  cm/s 
measured  1.0  cm  above  the  tops  of  Alcyonium  colonies. 
The  integrated  flow  over  the  colony  will  show  a  lower 
mean  value,  since  the  flow  speed  is  reduced  as  one  ap- 
proaches the  substrate  through  the  logarithmic  boundary 
layer  (Denny,  1988). 

Calculations  show  that  a  mean  particle  flux  of  0.04 
particles/fern2  •  s)  occurs  around  these  colonies  in  the  field, 
not  far  from  the  value  necessary  for  equality  of  the  han- 
dling time  and  filtration  time  for  Alcyonium  [0.01  parti- 
cles/(cnr  •  s)].  If  the  mean  flow  speed  is  ca.  1  cm/s,  the 
particle  flux  will  be  reduced  another  order  of  magnitude. 
Now  the  filtration  time  will  be  much  greater  than  the 
handling  time.  Under  such  conditions,  increases  in  the 
flow  speed  or  prey  concentration  will  cause  an  increase 
in  the  feeding  rate,  and  a  quasi-linear  response  will  be 
found,  similar  to  that  predicted  by  the  linear  model!  The 
model  described  in  the  Introduction  decomposes  to  the 
"linear"  model  of  passive  suspension  feeding  described 
above  when  the  particle  flux  past  the  organism  is  low. 
When  R2  (filtration  time)  is  large  compared  to  R,  (han- 
dling time),  a  becomes  almost  one.  The  second  term  of 
Eq.  (12)  goes  to  zero;  hence  prey  in  the  water  becomes 
prey  in  the  organism.  In  essence,  as  the  particle  flux  be- 
comes lower  (through  slower  flow  or  lower  prey  concen- 
trations), the  model  predicts  instantaneous  step  responses 


(capture)  of  single  plankters  or  100%  efficiency.  Are  field 
data  on  feeding  consistent  with  this  prediction  of  high 
feeding  efficiency? 

Sebens  and  Koehl  (1984)  sampled  gut  contents  of  the 
sea  anemone,  Melridium  and  Alcyonium  over  a  diel  cycle. 
Using  Sebens  (1984),  the  plankton  concentration  for  the 
site  averages  about  4  plankters/1  during  the  warmer 
months.  Assume  the  prey  inside  the  organisms  were 
caught  during  the  previous  two  hours  as  per  Sebens  and 
Koehl  ( 1 984).  Their  data  give  a  mean  number  of  prey  per 
colony  of  Alcyonium  (n  =  90).  Assume  each  Alcyonium 
colony  had  a  projected  surface  area  normal  to  the  flow 
capable  of  capturing  prey  of  10  cm2.  Given  the  above 
plankton  density,  and  an  efficiency  of  100%,  a  current  of 
2. 1  cm/s  would  be  needed  to  account  for  the  gut  contents. 
This  flow  speed  is  within  the  typical  range  of  speeds  seen 
above  these  organisms  (Table  I).  Of  course,  these  calcu- 
lations are  crude  estimates  because  ( 1 )  different  sizes  and 
types  of  plankton  are  lumped  in  particle  counts,  and  (2) 
both  species  prefer  certain  types  of  plankton  over  others. 
But  high  efficiencies  for  prey  capture  in  Alcyonium  seem 
reasonable  for  field  values  of  flow  and  prey  concentration 
(non-patch  conditions). 

Efficiencies  measured  under  very  high  particle  concen- 
trations in  the  flume  were  an  order  of  magnitude  lower 
than  these  field  estimates.  This  dichotomy  is  predicted  hy 
the  model:  at  very  high  plankton  concentrations,  feeding 
becomes  uncoupled  from  particle  flux;  under  field  con- 
ditions, efficiencies  skyrocket,  presumably  due  to  the  lower 
particle  flux  and  hence  favorable  (R:/R,)  ratio.  Why 
couldn't  these  feeding  experiments  be  repeated  in  the  lab- 
oratory using  particle  fluxes  representative  of  non-patch 
concentrations?  In  the  flume,  feeding  was  studied  at  high 
concentrations  over  short  period  of  time  for  two  reasons: 
( 1 )  concentration  and  hence  particle  flux  remained  con- 
stant in  the  flume  only  over  a  period  of  30  min;  after  that 
time,  gravitational  settlement  significantly  affects  con- 
centration, and  (2)  at  realistic  concentrations,  capture 
events  are  on  the  order  of  minutes  to  large  fractions  of 
an  hour  apart,  and  would  be  tedious  to  document,  even 
if  concentration  could  be  kept  constant.  Using  SCUBA, 
I  did  spend  several  hours  observing  colonies  of  Alcyonium 
feeding  in  situ  at  the  four  sites  sampled  for  flow  speed. 
Because  the  particles  on  which  they  feed  are  only  a  few 
hundred  micra  in  length,  this  requires  approaching  within 
30  cm  of  the  colony  to  observe  capture  events;  this  nec- 
essarily alters  the  flow  around  the  colony.  Only  rarely  in 
the  field  did  I  see  "rapid"  capture  of  prey  at  a  rate  com- 
parable to  that  seen  in  the  flume  (seconds  between  cap- 
tures); during  these  rare  events  there  was  an  easily  dis- 
cerned "cloud"  of  copepods  near  the  colonies.  However, 
most  of  the  time,  the  interval  between  prey  capture  events 
(visible  particle  adhesion  followed  by  movement  of  the 
tentacle  towards  the  pharynx)  was  several  minutes  in 


90 


M    R    PATTERSON 


length,  with  occasional  mind  (and  body)  numbing  pauses 
of  up  to  10  min  between  capture  events. 

An  examination  of  the  data  of  Sebens  and  Koehl  (1984) 
shows  that  even  under  the  best  conditions,  the  interval 
between  capture  events  must  be  over  a  minute  for  Al- 
cyonium and  2  min  for  Metridlum.  Barange  and  Gili 
( 1988)  sampled  the  coelenteron  contents  of  a  benthic  hy- 
droid  over  a  diel  cycle.  From  their  data  [mean  prey  items 
captured  per  (polyp  •  day),  number  of  polyps  per  colony], 
I  calculated  the  average  interval  between  capture  events 
to  be  about  1.3  min.  Thus,  passive  suspension  feeding  for 
these  organisms  is  a  slow  process  for  non-patch  concen- 
trations of  prey.  Cnidarian  colonies  snag  particles  slowly 
from  the  water  when  a  patch  isn't  around,  unlike  some 
vertebrate  suspension  feeders  that  capture  enormous 
quantities  of  particles  in  the  same  period  of  time  (Sand- 
erson and  Wassersug,  1990). 

Saturation  of  colonies  remains  a  [muling  phenomenon 

The  utility  of  this  model  is  that  it  points  out  some  new 
directions  for  work  with  passive  suspension  feeding  cni- 
darians.  An  unanswered  question  is  why  are  these  filters 
not  adapted  for  high  efficiency  nitration  under  high  par- 
ticle fluxes?  Is  there  a  biological  constraint  on  the  system 
that  limits  feeding?  Constraints  found  in  other  suspension 
feeding  systems  include  saturation  of  the  filter  (Parker, 
1975;  Real,  1977)  or  gut-filling  (Doyle,  1979).  Neither  of 
these  constraints  appears  likely  for  this  species.  Alcyonium 
colonies  began  slowing  their  feeding  rate  long  before  most 
polyps  had  successfully  fed  once.  They  are  also  capable 
of  packing  many  prey  items  into  a  single  polyp  (Patterson, 
1984).  Lasker  el  al.  (1982)  showed  that  in  single-polyped 
Hydra,  the  ingestion  of  prey  was  controlled  by  previous 
feeding  events,  i.e..  prey  captured  later  in  a  feeding  bout 
were  less  likely  to  be  ingested  than  prey  caught  near  the 
beginning.  Burnett  el  al.  ( 1 960)  and  Hand  ( 1 96 1 )  showed 
that  nematocyst  discharge  in  Hydra  is  inhibited  by  food 
in  the  gastrovascular  cavity,  and  Lasker  et  al.  ( 1 982)  spec- 
ulate that  this  may  be  important  in  limiting  ingestion  rate. 
But  Ruch  and  Cook  (1984)  have  demonstrated  inacti- 
vation  of  nematocyst  discharge  even  in  the  absence  of 
food  in  the  gut.  This  startling  observation  was  explored 
further  by  Clark  and  Cook  (1986)  using  a  colonial  hydroid. 
They  provide  evidence  from  lab  feeding  experiments  that 
the  accumulation  of  discharge  products  from  the  stenotele 
nematocysts  used  by  this  hydroid  in  prey  capture  is  suf- 
ficient to  inhibit  further  feeding,  and  that  it  is  not  necessary 
to  invoke  waste  product  accumulation  from  digestion,  or 
depletion  of  nematocysts,  to  explain  the  phenomenon. 
For  those  cnidarians  exhibiting  this  interesting  feedback, 
the  second  assumption  of  the  model  (see  Introduction) 
could  easily  be  reformulated  to  incorporate  a  term  spec- 
ifying the  diffusion  time  of  the  nematocyst  discharge 


products.  It  is  unknown  whether  nematocyst  discharge 
products  affect  Alcyonium  in  a  similar  fashion. 

The  nerve  net  is  also  probably  involved  in  the  process 
of  modulating  prey  capture  in  cnidarians  (McFarlane, 
1978).  Deformation  of  the  tentacle  by  repeated  particle 
impactions  may  be  important  in  producing  inhibition  of 
nematocyst  discharge  during  feeding  in  plankton  patches. 
On  a  larger  scale,  flow  induced  deformation  of  the  entire 
colony  may  be  important  in  regulating  the  rate  process 
of  prey  capture.  Best  ( 1988)  found  that  feeding  rate  in  a 
sea  pen,  Ptilosarcus,  increased  then  decreased  with  flow 
speed  and  attributed  this  behavior  to  changes  in  volume 
flow  rate  that  occurred  as  the  filtering  surfaces  changed 
their  orientation.  A  similar  phenomenon  was  noted  in  a 
crinoid  (Leonard  et  al..  1988). 

Some  experiments  that  would  help  solve  the  mystery 
of  why  colonies  saturate  long  before  all  filtering  units 
(polyps)  have  fed  would  include  ( 1 )  stealing  particles  from 
the  tentacles  after  capture  but  before  transfer  to  the  mouth, 
while  monitoring  frequency  of  capture  and  attempted 
ingestion  events,  (2)  eliciting  repeated  nematocyst  dis- 
charge by  micromolar  diffusion  clouds  of  amino  acids 
from  a  micropipette  near  tentacle  tips  or  mechanical 
stimulation  of  tentacles  while  the  cnidarian  colony  is  si- 
multaneously feeding,  (3)  separating  a  cnidarian  colony 
into  two  halves  except  for  a  strip  of  tissue  and  examining 
feeding  rates  in  the  two  halves  before  and  after  the  con- 
nection is  severed  [Clark  and  Cook  ( 1 986)  found  no  effect 
for  a  hydroid],  and  (4)  offering  digestible  and  non-digest- 
ible prey  to  a  species  that  will  ingest  both  types  of  particles 
(cf.  Lasker  ct  al..  1983)  and  measuring  feeding  rates  on 
both  types  of  particles  separately  and  together  while  ne- 
matocyst discharge  products  are  monitored. 

It  is  very  intriguing  that  this  colonial  octocoral  saturates 
after  a  few  minutes  of  feeding  in  high  prey  densities  at 
about  the  same  number  of  prey  that  would  be  caught  over 
a  2-4  h  period  in  the  field  (Sebens  and  Koehl,  1984). 
Digestion  of  prey  items  renders  them  unidentifiable  after 
4-6  h  (Sebens  and  Koehl,  1984).  Have  these  colonial  sus- 
pension feeders  evolved  to  "charge  their  capacitance"  on 
a  time  scale  of  approximately  two  hours  because  they  are 
limited  by  the  activity  of  their  digestive  enzymes?  For 
boreal  cnidarians  in  the  Atlantic,  the  strongest  tidal  flows 
will  be  obtained  for  a  2-4  h  period  between  slack  tides. 
Because  plankton  patches  are  the  exception  rather  than 
the  rule,  the  feeding  response  may  have  evolved  to  cope 
with  sparse  prey  moving  past  the  colony  over  a  2-4  h 
period.  During  periods  of  flow  dominated  by  wave-driven 
oscillations,  e.g.,  slack  tides,  colonies  will  re-filter  water 
already  low  in  prey.  Feeding  in  a  bi-directional  flow  can 
actually  increase  feeding  success  in  a  hydroid  exposed  to 
high  (patch)  concentration  of  prey  (Hunter,  1989).  At 
present,  it  is  unknown  for  this  species  whether  feeding 


MODEL  OF  PASSIVE  SUSPENSION  FEEDING 


91 


effectiveness  is  higher  in  bi-directional  flow  at  low  prey 
concentrations. 

Application  of  this  model  to  other  passive  suspension 
feeders  will  test  its  generality  and  provide  evidence  for 
whether  the  dichotomy  in  feeding  behavior  characteristic 
of  this  species  when  feeding  in  low  and  high  prey  con- 
centrations is  a  widespread  phenomenon.  Future  devel- 
opments in  the  measurement  and  description  of  plankton 
patchiness  on  a  small  scale  in  nearshore  waters  (Pieper 
and  Holliday,  1985)  and  description  of  the  benthic 
boundary  layer  in  which  these  organisms  live  (Jumars 
and  Nowell,  1984)  will  improve  our  ability  to  model  and 
understand  this  fascinating  process. 

Acknowledgments 

Discussions  with  P.  Basser,  B.  Best,  R.  Etter,  T.  Givnish, 
R.  Grosberg,  S.  Kleinhaus,  W.  McFarland,  T.  McMahon, 
R.  Olson,  S.  L.  Sanderson,  K.  Sebens,  R.  Turner,  the 
HUMP  seminar  group,  and  the  Aquatic  Sciences  group 
at  M.  I.  T.,  have  improved  the  paper  greatly.  P.  Rudy, 
Acting  Director,  Marine  Science  Center,  Northeastern 
University.  Nahant,  Massachusetts,  kindly  provided  lab- 
oratory space.  C.  Alexander  and  B.  Otteson  provided 
technical  assistance.  Financial  support  for  this  project  was 
provided  by  the  Richmond  Fund  of  Harvard  University, 
the  Lerner-Gray  Fund  for  Marine  Research  of  the  Amer- 
ican Museum  of  Natural  History,  NSF  OCE-8308958  to 
K.  Sebens  of  Harvard  University,  a  Faculty  Research 
Award  from  UC  Davis,  and  NSF  OCE-87 16427  to  the 
author. 

Literature  Cited 

Alldredge,  A.  A.,  and  J.  M.  King.  1977.     Distribution,  abundance,  and 

substrate  preferences  of  demersal  reef  zooplankton  at  Lizard  Is.,  Great 

Barrier  Reef.  Mar.  Biol.  41:  317-334. 
Barange,  M.,  and  J.  M.  Gili,  1988.     Feeding  cycles  and  prey  capture 

in  Eudendniim  racemosum  (Cavolini,  1785).  J.  Exp.  Mar.  Biol.  Ecol. 

115:  281-293. 
Best,  B.  A.  1988.     Passive  suspension  feeding  in  a  sea  pen:  effects  of 

ambient  flow  on  volume  flow  rate  and  filtering  efficiency.  Biol.  Bull. 

175:  332-342. 
Burnett,  A.  L.,  T.  Lenlz,  and  M.  Warren.  1960.     The  question  of  control 

of  the  nematocyst  discharge  reaction  by  fully  fed  hydra.  Ann.  Soc.. 

Roy  Zoo/  Big.  90:  247-268. 
Clark,  S.  D.,  and  C.  B.  Cook.  1986.     Inhibition  of  nematocyst  discharge 

during  feeding  in   the  colonial   hydroid   Halocordy/e  disticha 

(=Pennaria  tiarella):  the  role  of  previous  prey  killing.  Bin!.  Bull  171: 

405-416. 
Clayton.  VV.  S.,  Jr.,  and  H.  R.  Lasker,  1982.     Effects  of  light  and  dark 

treatments  on  feeding  by  the  reef  coral  Pocilloporu  damicornts  (Lin- 
naeus). J  E\p.  Mar  Biol.  Ecol.  63:  269-279. 
Coughlan,  J.  1969.     The  estimation  of  filtering  rate  from  the  clearance 

of  suspensions.  Mar.  Biol.  2:  356-358. 
Crowell,  S.  1957.     Differential  responses  of  growth  zones  to  nutritive 

level,  age  and  temperature  in  the  colonial  hydroid,  Campanularia. 

J  Exp.  Zoo/.  134:  63-90. 


Denny,  M.  1988.     Biology  ami  I  he  Mechanics  of  I  he  Wave-Swept  En- 
vironment. Princeton  University  Press,  Princeton. 
Dorman,  R.  G.   1966.     Filtration.   Pp.    195-222  in  Aerosol  Science. 

C.  N.  Davies,  ed.  Academic  Press,  New  York. 

Doyle,  R.  \V.  1979.     Ingestion  rate  of  a  selective  deposit  feeder  in  a 
complex  mixture  of  particles:  testing  the  energy-optimization  hy- 
pothesis. Limnol.  Oceanogr.  24:  867-874. 
Emlen,  J.  M.  1973.     Ecology  .In  Evolutionary  Approach.  Addison- 

Wesley,  Reading.  Massachusetts. 
Kasham,  M.  J.  R.  1978.     The  statistical  and  mathematical  analysis  of 

plankton  patchiness.  Oceanogr.  Mar.  Biol.  Ann.  Rev.  16:  43-79. 
Fiala-Medioni,  A.  1973.     Ethologie  alimentaire  d'invertebres  benthiques 
filtreurs  (ascidies).  I.  Dispotif  experimental.  Taux  de  filtration  et  de 
digestion  chez  Phallusni  mammillata  Mar.  Biol.  23:  137-145. 
Fiala-Medioni,  A.  I978a.     Filter  feeding  ethology  of  benthic  invertebrates 
(ascidians).  III.  Recording  of  water  current  in  stilt-rate  and  rhythm 
of  pumping.  Mar  Biol  45:  185-140. 

Fiala-Medioni,  A.  I978b.  Filter  feeding  ethology  of  benthic  inverte- 
brates (ascidians).  IV.  Pumping  rate,  filtration  rate,  filtration  efficiency. 
Mar.  Biol.  48:  243-249. 

Fiala-Medioni,  A.  1978c.  Filter  feeding  ethology  of  benthic  invertebrates 
(ascidians).  V.  Influence  of  temperature  on  pumping,  filtration  and 
digestion  rates  and  rhythms  in  Phallusia  mammillata.  Mar  Biol  48: 
251-259. 

Fiala-Medioni,  A.  1978d.  A  scanning  electron  microscope  study  of  the 
branchial  sac  of  benthic  filter-feeding  invertebrates  (ascidians).  Ada 
Zoo/.  (Stockholm)  59:  1-10. 

Grosberg,  R.  K.  1982.  Intertidal  zonation  of  barnacles:  the  influence 
of  planktonic  zonation  of  larvae  on  vertical  distribution  of  adults. 
Ecology  63(4):  894-899. 

Hand,  C.  1961.  Present  state  of  nematocyst  research  types,  structure, 
and  function.  Pp.  187-202  in  The  Biology  of  Hydra.  H.  M.  LenhofT 
and  W.  F.  Loomis,  eds.  Univ.  of  Miami  Press,  Miami. 
1  lolling,  C.  S.  1965.  The  functional  response  of  predators  to  prey  density 
and  its  role  in  mimicry  and  population  regulation.  Mem  Enlontol 
Soc.  Canada  No.  45,  60  pp. 

Hunter,  T.  1989.     Suspension  feeding  in  oscillating  flow:  the  effect  of 
colony  morphology  and  flow  regime  on  plankton  capture  by  the  hy- 
droid Obelta  longissima  Biol  Bull  176:  41-49. 
Jergensen,  C.  B.  1943.     On  the  water  transport  through  the  gills  of 

bivalves.  Ada  Physiol.  Scam/  5:  277-304. 
Jergensen,  C.  B.  1966.     Biology  o/  Suspension  Feeding.  Pergamon  Press, 

Oxford. 
Jorgensen,  C.  B.  1975.     On  gill  function  in  the  mussel  Mvtilus  edulix 

L.  Ophelia  13:  187-232. 

Jumars,  P.  A.,  and  A.  R.  M.  Nowell.  1984.     Fluid  and  sediment  dynamic 
effects  on  marine  benthic  community  structure.  Am.  Zool.  24:  45- 
55. 
La  Bar  her  a.  M.,  and  S.  Vogel.  1976.  An  inexpensive  thermistor  flowmeter 

for  aquatic  biology.  Limnol.  Oceanogr.  21:  750-756. 
Lam,  R.  K.,  and  B.  VV.  Frost.  1976.     Model  of  copepod  filtering  response 
to  changes  in  size  and  concentration  of  food.  Limnol.  Oceanogr  21: 
490-500. 

Lasker,  H.  R.  1976.  Intraspecific  variability  of  zooplankton  feeding  in 
the  hermatypic  coral  Montastrea  cavernosa.  Pp.  101-109  in  Coel- 
en/erate  Ecology  and  Behavior.  C.  O.  Mackie,  ed.  Plenum,  New  York. 
Lasker,  H.  R.,  J.  A.  Syron,  and  W.  S.  Clayton,  Jr.  1982.  The  feeding 
response  of  Hydra  vindis  effects  of  prey  density  on  capture  rates. 
Biol.  Bull  162:  290-298. 

Lasker,  H.  R.,  M.  D.  Gottfried,  and  M.  A.  Coffroth.  1983.  Effects  of 
depth  on  the  feeding  capabilities  of  two  octocorals.  Mar.  Biol.  73: 
73-78. 

Lehman,  J.  T.  1976.  The  filter-feeder  as  an  optimal  forager,  and  the 
predicted  shapes  of  feeding  curves.  Limnol.  Oceanogr.  21 :  50 1  -5 1 6. 


92 


M.  R.  PATTERSON 


Leonard,  A.  B.,  J.  R.  Strickler,  and  N.  D.  Holland.  1988.  Effects  of 
current  speed  on  filtration  during  suspension  feeding  in  Oligometra 
serripinna  (Echinodermala:  Crinoidea).  Mar.  Biol.  97:  1 1  1-122. 

Leversee,  G.  J.  1976.  Flow  and  feeding  in  fan-shaped  colonies  of  a 
gorgonian  coral.  l.eptogorgia.  Bio/.  Bull  151:  344-356. 

McFarlane,  I.  D.  1978.  Multiple  conducting  systems  and  the  control 
of  behaviour  in  the  brain  coral  Mcandnna  meandriles  (L.).  Proc. 
Roy.  Sac.  Lond  B.  200:  193-216. 

MiKinii.  J.  II.  1966.  Biological  Control  Systems  Analysis.  McGraw- 
Hill,  New  York. 

Mohlenberg,  F.,  and  H.  U.  Riisgard.  1978.  Efficiency  of  particle  re- 
tention in  13  species  of  suspension  feeding  bivalves.  Ophelia  17:  239- 
246. 

Muscatine.  L.,  and  J.  W.  Porter.  1977.  Reef  corals:  mutualistic  sym- 
bioses  adapted  to  nutrient-poor  environments.  BioScience  27(7):  454- 
460. 

Okamura,  B.  1984.  The  effects  of  ambient  flow  velocity,  colony  size, 
and  upstream  colonies  on  the  feeding  sucess  of  Bryozoa.,  I.  Buguhi 
slolonifera  Ryland,  an  arborescent  species.  /  E.\p.  Mar.  Biol.  Ecol. 
83:  179-193. 

Okubo,  A.  1980.  Diffusion  and  Ecological  Problems:  Mathematical 
Models.  (Biomathematics,  Vol.  10).  Springer  Verlag,  New  York. 

Ortner,  P.  B.,  L.  C.  Hill,  and  H.  E.  Edgerton.  1981.  In-sitii  silhouette 
photography  of  Gulf  Stream  zooplankton.  Deep-Sea  Res.  28A(12): 
1569-1576. 

Ortner.  P.  B.,  R.  L.  Ferguson,  S.  R.  Piotrowicz,  L.  Chesal,  G.  Berberian, 
and  A.  V.  Palumbo.  1984.  Biological  consequences  of  hydrographic 
and  atmospheric  advection  within  the  Gulf  Loop  Intrusion.  Deep- 
Sea  Res.  31(9):  1  101-1  120. 

Palmer,  R.,  and  L.  Williams.  1980.  Effect  of  particle  concentration  on 
filtration  efficiency  of  the  Bay  Scallop  Argopeclen  irradians  and  the 
oyster  Crassoslrea  virginica.  Ophelia  19:  163-174. 

Parker,  M.  1975.  Similarities  between  the  uptake  of  nutrients  and  the 
ingestion  of  prey.  \'crh.  Internal,  herein.  Limnol  19:  56-59. 

Patterson,  M.  R.  1980.  Hydromechanical  adaptations  in  Alcyonium 
sideriinn  (Octocorallia).  Pp.  183-201  in  Biojluid  Mechanics  2,  D.  J. 
Schneck.  ed.  Plenum,  New  York. 

Patterson,  M.  R.  1984.  Patterns  of  whole  colony  prey  capture  in  the 
octocoral,  Alcyonium  sidenum.  Biol.  Bull.  167:  613-629. 

Patterson,  M.  R.,  and  K.  P.  Sebens.  1989.  Forced  convection  modulates 
gas  exchange  in  cnidarians.  Proc.  Nat.  Acad.  Sci.  (USA)  86:  8833- 
8836. 

Pieper,  R.  E.,  and  D.  V.  Holliday.  1985.  Acoustic  measurements  of 
zooplankton  distributions  in  the  sea.  J.  Cons.  Cons.  Int.  Explor.  Mer 
41(3):  226-238. 


Real,  L.  A.  1977.  The  kinetics  of  functional  response.  Am.  Nalur  111: 
284-300. 

Robbins,  R.  E.,  and  J.  M.  Snick,  1980.  Expansion-contraction  behavior 
in  the  sea  anemone  Meiridium  senile:  environmental  cues  and  ener- 
getic consequences.  Pp.  101-1 16  in  Nutrition  in  the  Lower  Metazoa, 
D.  C.  Smith  and  Y.  Tiffon.  eds.  Pergamon,  New  York. 

Rubenstein,  D.  J..  and  M.  A.  R.  Koehl.  1977.  The  mechanism  of  filter 
feeding:  some  theoretical  considerations.  Am.  Nalur.  Ill:  981-994. 

Ruch,  R.  J.,  and  C.  B.  Cook.  1984.  Nematocyst  inactivation  during 
feeding  in  Hydra  liltoralis  J  E.\p.  Biol.  Ill:  31-42. 

Sanderson,  S.  L.,  and  R.  Wassersug.  1990.  Suspension-feeding  verte- 
brates. Sci.  Am.  262:  96-101. 

Seale,  D.  B.  1982.  Obligate  and  facultative  suspension  feeding  in  anuran 
larvae:  feeding  regulation  in  Xenoptis  and  Rana.  Biol  Bull  162:  214- 
231. 

Sebens,  K.  P.  1979.  The  energetics  of  asexual  reproduction  and  colony 
formation  in  benthic  marine  invertebrates.  Am.  Zoo/.  19:  683-697. 

Sebens,  K.  P.  1984.  Water  flow  and  coral  colony  size:  interhabitat 
comparisons  of  the  octocoral  Alcyonium  sidenum.  Proc  Natl.  Aead. 
Sci  (USA)  81:  5473-5477. 

Sebens,  K.  P.,  and  M.  A.  R.  Koehl.  1984.  Predation  on  zooplankton 
by  the  benthic  anthozoans,  Alcyonium  sidenum  (Alcyonacea)  and 
Metridiwn  senile  (Actiniaria)  in  the  New  England  subtidal.  Mar. 
Biol  81:255-271. 

Sebens,  K.  P.  1986.  Spatial  relationships  among  encrusting  manne 
organisms  in  the  New  England  subtidal  zone.  Ecol.  Monogr.  56:  73- 
96. 

Sokal,  R.  R.,  and  F.  J.  Rohlf.  1981.  Biometry.  W.  H.  Freeman,  San 
Francisco. 

Szmant-Froelich,  A.,  and  M.  E.  Q.  Pilson.  1984.  Effects  of  feeding 
frequency  and  symbiosis  with  zooxanthellae  on  nitrogen  metabolism 
and  respiration  of  the  coral  Astrangia  danae.  Alar.  Biol.  81:  153- 
162. 

Townsend,  C.  R.,  and  R.  N.  Hughes.  1981.  Maximizing  net  energy 
returns  from  foraging.  Pp.  86-108  in  Physiological  Ecology:  An  Evo- 
lutionary Approach.  C.  R.  Townsend  and  P.  Calow.  eds.  Sinauer 
Associates.  Sunderland,  Massachusetts. 

Wetzel,  R.  G.  1975.     Limnology  W.  B.  Saunders.  Philadelphia. 

Wiebe,  P.  H.  1970.  Small-scale  spatial  distribution  in  oceanic  zoo- 
plankton.  Limnol.  Oceanogr.  15:  205-217. 

Wiebe,  P.  H.  1971.  A  computer  model  study  of  zooplankton  patchiness 
and  its  effects  on  sampling  error.  Limnol.  Oceanogr.  16:  29-38. 

Williams,  L.  G.  1982.  Mathematical  analysis  of  the  effects  of  particle 
retention  efficiency  on  determination  of  filtration  rate.  Mar.  Biol. 
66:  171-177. 


Reference:  Bwl  Bull  180:  93-102.  (February,  1991) 


The  Effects  of  Flow  on  Polyp-Level  Prey  Capture  in  an 
Octocoral,  Alcyonium  siderium 

MARK  R.  PATTERSON 

Division  of  Environmental  Studies,  University  of  California,  Davis.  California  95616 


Abstract.  Particle  capture  by  individual  polyps  and  ten- 
tacles of  the  octocoral,  Alcyonium  siderium,  was  investi- 
gated in  flows  of  different  speed  and  turbulence  intensity. 
In  low  flow  (Umean  =  2.1  cm/s;  u'  =  1.2  cm/s.  where  u'  is 
the  root  mean  square  of  the  fluctuations  from  Umean),  ten- 
tacles on  the  upstream  side  of  a  polyp  capture  the  most 
prey.  In  intermediate  flow  (Umean  =  12.2  cm/s;  u'  =  6.0 
cm/s).  downstream  tentacles  within  a  polyp  catch  the  most 
prey.  In  high  flow  (Umcan  =  19.8  cm/s;  u'  =  4.0  cm/s), 
polyps  are  bent  downstream,  eddies  form  over  the  ten- 
tacular surfaces,  and  the  capture  distribution  over  tentacles 
becomes  radially  symmetric.  At  all  flow  speeds  tested, 
particles  are  caught  with  increasing  frequency  nearer  the 
tip  of  the  tentacle  relative  to  locations  near  the  pharynx. 
At  the  highest  flow  speed  tested,  no  particles  are  caught 
on  the  segment  of  each  tentacle  closest  to  the  pharynx. 
The  per  polyp  capture  efficiency  is  low  and  drops  mark- 
edly with  increasing  Reynolds  number.  The  capture 
mechanism  for  this  species  appears  to  be  direct  intercep- 
tion; inertial  impaction  is  shown  to  be  unimportant.  Flow 
modulation  of  particle  capture  by  polyps  is  probably  a 
general  phenomenon  among  octocorals. 

Introduction 

The  application  of  modern  engineering  theory  to  the 
analysis  of  particle  capture  by  suspension  feeding  organ- 
isms began  with  a  review  of  the  engineering  and  fluid 
mechanics  literature  by  Rubenstein  and  Koehl  (1977). 
The  medical  community  has  long  been  interested  in  the 
related  problem  of  how  particles  are  deposited  in  the  tra- 
cheobronchial  tree  of  mammalian  lungs  (Findeisen,  1935; 
Landahl,  1950,  1963).  The  theory  invoked  was  the  same 
(McMahon  el  a/..  1977),  and  importance  of  flow  pattern 
in  affecting  the  location  of  particle  capture  inside  lungs 

Received  30  August  1989;  accepted  6  November  1990. 


was  recognized  (Bell,  1974).  Particle  capture  mechanisms 
used  by  biological  niters  include  direct  interception  (the 
particle  comes  within  one  particle  radius  of  a  filtering  sur- 
face), inertial  impaction  (the  particle  contacts  the  filter 
because  particle  inertia  causes  it  to  deviate  from  a  fluid 
streamline  around  the  filter),  diffusive  deposition  (both 
Brownian  and  eddy-enhanced — the  motion  of  the  particle 
across  streamlines  leads  to  contact  with  the  filter),  sieving 
(the  particle  is  too  large  to  pass  through  gaps  in  the  filter 
geometry),  electrostatic  attraction  (requires  net  surface 
charges  of  opposite  sign  on  filter  and  particle),  and  grav- 
itational deposition  (a  particle  denser  than  the  fluid  sinks 
and  contacts  the  filter).  Aerosol  filtration  theory  allows 
estimation  of  the  relative  importance  of  these  mechanisms 
by  calculating  the  ratios  of  relevant  forces  acting  on  par- 
ticles of  different  charge,  size,  and  density,  in  flows  of 
different  velocity  and  viscosity  (LaBarbera,  1984). 

Recent  work  in  organisms  as  diverse  as  conifers  (Niklas, 
1982a.b),  dipteran  larvae  (Ross  and  Craig,  1980),  poly- 
chaetes  (Taghon  el  a/..  1980;  Merz,  1984),  veliger  larvae 
of  mollusks  (Strathmann  and  Liese,  1979),  ophiuroids 
(LaBarbera,  1978),  bryozoans  (Okamura,  1984,  1987), 
hydroids  (Harvell  and  LaBarbera,  1985;  Hunter,  1989), 
and  crinoids  (Holland  el  al.  1987)  has  extended  our 
knowledge  of  how  biological  filters  work  and  how  flow 
speed  can  affect  the  geometric  pattern  of  particle  capture 
at  the  level  of  the  colony  or  individual.  An  important 
generalization  from  these  works  is  that  the  coupling  be- 
tween an  organism's  filter  morphology  and  the  resulting 
flow  field,  whether  generated  actively  or  passively,  is  ex- 
tremely important  in  the  particle  capture  process. 

Few  studies  have  examined  the  mechanics  of  zoo- 
plankton  capture  by  passive  suspension  feeding  cnidarians 
at  the  level  of  the  filtering  elements  themselves,  the  polyps 
composed  of  tentacles.  Lewis  and  Price  (1976)  investigated 
mucus  feeding  in  corals.  Hunter  (1989)  demonstrated  that 
feeding  effectiveness  of  hydroid  polyps  was  greater  in  os- 


93 


94 


M.  R.  PATTERSON 


dilatory  flow  compared  to  steady,  unidirectional  flow. 
Harvell  and  LaBarbera  (1985)  examined  how  flexibility 
affects  local  flow  speeds  over  polyps  in  hydroid  colonies. 
Best  (1988)  has  investigated  prey  capture  in  the  sea  pen, 
Ptilosarcus gumeyi,  a  species  that  has  the  polyps  arranged 
at  the  ends  of  cantilevered  support  tissue  through  which 
flowing  seawater  passes.  She  found  that  the  deformability 
of  the  organism  strongly  affected  nitration  efficiency  and 
volume  of  water  filtered;  in  effect  the  organism  could  tune 
its  feeding  performance  by  maintaining  a  variable  porosity 
filter. 

While  some  workers  have  investigated  the  location  of 
prey  capture  on  the  surface  of  cnidarian  colonies  (Lev- 
ersee,  1976;  Lasker,  1981;  Patterson,  1984),  no  studies 
have  addressed  the  location  of  prey  capture  within  indi- 
vidual polyps  or  tentacles.  Alcyonium  siderium  Verrill  is 
a  planktivorous  colonial  octocoral  common  on  hard  rock 
substrate  in  the  New  England  subtidal  (Sebens,  1986).  Its 
diet  consists  largely  of  small  zooplankton;  invertebrate 
eggs,  foraminiferans,  ascidian  larvae,  nematodes,  harpac- 
ticoid  and  calanoid  copepods,  barnacle  nauplii  and  cy- 
prids,  ostracods.  and  crustacean  fragments  were  common, 
although  algal  material  is  often  present  (Sebens  and  Koehl, 
1984).  It  readily  captures  and  eats  live  zooplankton  and 
Artemia  cysts  in  a  laboratory  flume  (Patterson,  1984).  Prey 
capture  events  by  individual  tentacles  are  easily  observed 
by  the  naked  eye.  The  subtidal  habitats  it  occupies  can 
differ  greatly  in  water  motion  (Sebens,  1984;  Patterson, 
1985),  and  there  is  a  change  in  colony  morphology  of 
local  populations  that  is  correlated  with  flow  regime,  with 
finger-like  colonies  found  in  slower  flow  areas,  and  lobate 
ellipsoids  and  roughly  spherical  forms  found  in  higher 
flows  (Patterson,  1980).  The  present  study  tested  whether 
the  location  of  prey  capture  within  individual  polyps  and 
tentacles  of  the  boreal  octocoral,  Alcyonium  siderium.  is 
affected  by  flow  patterns  over  the  colony. 

Materials  and  Methods 

Colony  collection,  maintenance,  and  flow  generation 
and  measurement 

Polyp  feeding  experiments  were  conducted  at  the  Ma- 
rine Science  Center  (MSC),  Northeastern  University,  Na- 
hant,  Massachusetts,  and  at  the  University  of  California, 
Davis.  Colonies  of  A.  siderium  were  collected  and  main- 
tained in  flowing  seawater  tables  or  recirculating  chilled 
aquaria.  A  recirculating  Plexiglas  flume  (98  liters;  15  cm 
X  15  cm  working  section;  1.9  m  long)  patterned  after  a 
design  published  by  Vogel  and  LaBarbera  (1978)  and 
equipped  with  a  chiller  (Aquanetics)  enabled  colonies  to 
feed  in  flows  of  various  speeds  and  turbulence  intensities 
at  15°C.  Patterson  (1984)  gives  a  quantitative  description 
of  flow  in  this  particular  flume.  All  experiments  were  per- 
formed with  the  flow  straighteners  removed,  resulting  in 


higher  turbulence  in  the  flow  tank  and  a  closer  approxi- 
mation to  the  flow  seen  over  the  colonies  in  the  field  (Pat- 
terson and  Sebens,  1989).  Seawater  for  the  flume  was  ob- 
tained from  the  MSC  seawater  system  and  was  filtered 
twice  (sand,  cotton  mesh)  to  remove  particles  greater  than 
20  jim  diameter  or  was  made  fresh  using  InstantOcean, 
and  adjusted  to  a  salinity  of  34%o. 

Flow  speeds  and  turbulence  intensities  were  measured 
with  a  two  channel  thermistor  flowmeter  circuit  modified 
from  LaBarbera  and  Vogel  (1976).  The  frequency  re- 
sponse of  the  probe  plus  circuit  is  5  Hz  at  —6  dB  down 
from  maximum  response.  The  turbulence  intensities  may 
underestimate  the  true  turbulence  in  the  flume,  if  there 
is  significant  energy  at  frequencies  above  5  Hz;  this  aspect 
of  flume  flow  regime  was  not  evaluated.  The  velocity  signal 
was  converted  into  an  FM  signal  for  transmission  over  a 
distance  of  several  meters  to  a  frequency-to-voltage  (f/v) 
converter.  The  output  of  the  f/v  converter  was  sent  to  a 
signal  conditioner  (custom-made)  and  eight  bit  successive 
approximation  A/D  converter  (Mountain  Computer) 
connected  to  an  Apple  He  microcomputer.  The  sampling 
rate  was  10  Hz. 

Octocoral  colonies  attached  to  mussels  (Modiolus  mo- 
diolus)  were  collected  subtidally.  Mussel  shell  fragments 
bearing  Alcyonium  were  mounted  securely  in  the  flow 
tank  working  section.  The  prey  offered  to  the  colonies 
were  cysts  of  the  brine  shrimp,  Artemia.  Characteristics 
of  the  cysts  are  described  in  Patterson  (1984).  The  cysts 
are  about  the  same  size  as  the  mean  prey  size  taken  by 
Alcyonium  in  the  field  (Sebens  and  Koehl,  1984). 

Location  oj  prey  capture 

The  spatial  location  of  prey  capture  on  individual  po- 
lyps of  Alcyonium  was  studied  in  flows  of  high  turbulence 
levels  and  differing  mean  flow  speeds.  Colonies  used 
ranged  from  2  to  8  cm  in  greatest  dimension  when  ex- 
panded. Colonies  were  all  roughly  spherical.  Prior  to  an 
observation  period,  a  single  colony  was  introduced  to  the 
flow  tank  and  allowed  to  acclimate  to  the  flow  regime 
and  expand  its  polyps.  A  standard  volume  of  seawater 
(11)  and  weight  of  hydrated  cysts  (0.45  g)  were  added  to 
the  flow  tank  at  the  beginning  of  the  observation  period. 
The  flow  1  cm  above  the  observed  polyp  was  adjusted  to 
have  a  mean  value  of  2.7,  12.2,  or  19.8  cm/s  (4  s  average 
achieved  through  an  electronic  integrator)  by  adjusting 
the  speed  control  on  the  flume  motor.  Five  minutes  into 
the  experiment,  three  60-ml  samples  were  withdrawn  iso- 
kinetically  (Brodkey  and  Hershey,  1988)  using  a  Cole- 
Parmer  peristaltic  pump  (model  no.  7568)  smoothed  with 
hydraulic  capacitors.  Each  sample  was  filtered  onto  grid- 
ded  Millipore  filters,  the  cysts  counted,  and  the  mean 
concentration  of  particles  calculated.  The  range  of  particle 
concentrations  encountered  by  the  colony  among  exper- 
iments ranged  from  0.13  to  0.53  cysts/ml. 


POLYP-LEVEL  PREY  CAPTURE  EVENTS 


95 


Figure  1A  shows  a  typical  top  view  of  a  polyp  after  a 
feeding  bout  and  the  coordinate  system  used  to  assess 
location  of  capture  around  the  polyp's  circumference. 
Note  that  the  coordinate  system  for  the  top  projections 
paired  tentacles  from  the  bilaterally  symmetric  halves  of 
the  polyp  (Fig.  1A).  The  coordinate  variable  (x)  used  for 
describing  capture  along  individual  tentacles  is  shown  in 
Figure  IB;  note  that  the  distance  along  the  tentacle  (x)  is 
normalized  to  the  tentacle  length  (L).  Capture  events  on 
individual  polyps  were  observed  at  a  magnification  of  35 X 
through  a  dissecting  microscope  suspended  over  the  flume. 
A  watch  glass  floating  on  the  water  and  anchored  over 
the  colony  prevented  blurring  of  the  image  from  capillary 
waves  at  the  air/water  interface.  As  a  feeding  bout  pro- 
gressed, composite  maps  of  capture  sites  of  cysts  from 
individual  polyps  were  made  (Fig.  1A).  All  polyps  cen- 
sused  were  located  near  the  top  of  the  colony;  polyps  cho- 
sen for  observation  had  their  tentacles  oriented  to  the  flow 
(Fig.  1A). 

An  ocular  micrometer  permitted  measurement  of  rel- 
ative position  of  capture  along  the  length  of  a  tentacle. 
Only  particles  caught  on  the  oral  side  of  the  tentacles  were 
noted;  particles  were  very  rarely  captured  on  the  aboral 
side  of  the  tentacles.  The  tentacle  capture  maps  were  di- 
vided into  five  non-dimensional  length  sectors.  Particle 
counts  in  the  tentacle  length  sectors  were  normalized 
within  a  tentacle  to  the  projected  area  available  for  particle 
capture.  Surface  areas  of  polyps  were  computed  using  a 
camera  lucida  and  an  interactive  digitizing  tablet  (Apple 
Graphics  Tablet).  The  area  of  the  pinnules  was  not  mea- 
sured in  calculating  areas  available  for  capture.  Projected 
surface  area  of  the  entire  colony  was  measured  similarly. 

Feeding  efficiency 

A  dimensionless  measure  of  feeding  effectiveness,  the 
efficiency  of  prey  capture  at  the  polyp  level  was  computed 
as  follows:  the  number  of  particles  caught  per  polyp  during 
a  standard  feeding  bout  was  divided  by  the  number  of 
particles  passing  through  the  cross-sectional  area  occupied 
by  the  colony  (divided  by  the  number  of  polyps),  if  the 
colony  were  not  there.  This  result  is  the  standard  definition 
of  filtration  efficiency  from  engineering  theory  (Dorman, 
1966).  The  number  passing  through  the  cross-sectional 
area  was  calculated  by  integrating  the  flow  over  the  height 
and  width  of  the  colony  and  multiplying  by  the  sampled 
particle  concentration.  Feeding  rate  in  the  dense  concen- 
trations of  prey  used  in  the  flume  is  not  constant,  but 
decreases  with  time  (Patterson,  1991).  These  concentra- 
tions were  similar  to  zooplankton  patch  concentrations 
in  the  field.  Hence,  efficiency  is  a  function  of  time.  For 
purposes  of  comparison,  efficiency  was  computed  over 
the  time  necessary  to  reach  saturation.  Saturation  is  de- 
fined as  the  point  at  which  capture  events  drop  to  less 
than  one  prey  item  caught  per  5-min  period  per  colony. 


Results 

Prey  capture  around  the  periphery  of  a  polyp 

Figure  1C  shows  the  distribution  of  particle  capture 
around  the  polyps  on  the  different  tentacles  as  one  moves 
in  the  downstream  direction;  the  histograms  have  been 
adjusted  for  the  amount  of  surface  area  available  for  prey 
capture.  At  low  flow  speeds  (Umean  =  2.7  cm/s),  prey  cap- 
ture is  remarkably  asymmetric,  with  upstream  tentacles 
capturing  the  most  prey  [Kolmogorov-Smirnov  (K-S)  test; 
P  <  0.001 ;  df  =  207].  At  intermediate  flow  speeds  (Umean 
=  12.2  cm/s),  the  distribution  is  again  asymmetric,  but 
in  the  opposite  direction,  with  downstream  tentacles  fa- 
vored in  particle  capture  (K-S  test;  P  <  0.001;  df  =  205). 
At  the  highest  flow  speed  tested  (Umean  =  19.8  cm/s),  the 
distribution  is  indistinguishable  from  an  even  distribution 
of  prey  capture  around  the  tentacles  (K-S  test;  P  >  0.5; 
df  =  70).  The  distribution  of  capture  events  is  thus  mark- 
edly affected  by  flow  speed.  Only  at  the  highest  speeds 
can  capture  be  modeled  by  a  Poisson  process. 

Prey  capture  by  tentacles 

When  prey  capture  events  are  examined  over  the  length 
of  individual  tentacles,  an  additional  pattern  emerges  (Fig. 
2).  At  low  speeds  (Umcan  =  2.7  cm/s),  the  distribution  is 
significantly  asymmetric,  with  the  outer  segments  of  the 
tentacles  capturing  the  most  prey  (K-S  test;  P  <  0.001;  df 
=  207).  The  same  pattern  is  found  at  intermediate  flow 
speeds  (Umcan  =  12.2  cm/s),  with  the  asymmetry  shifted 
even  further  in  the  direction  away  from  the  pharynx  (K- 
S  test;  P  <  0.00 1 ;  df  =  205).  Finally,  at  the  highest  speed 
used  in  the  flume  (Umt.an  =  19.8  cm/s),  the  distribution  is 
still  asymmetric  with  a  bias  toward  the  tentacle  tips  (K-S 
test;  P  <  0.001 ;  df  =  70),  but  with  some  differences.  Cap- 
ture in  the  innermost  20%  of  the  tentacle's  length  has 
disappeared,  and  the  outermost  20%  experiences  much 
more  variable  prey  capture  success. 

Feeding  efficiency 

When  the  efficiency  of  filtration  for  individual  polyps 
is  examined  with  respect  to  Reynolds  number  (Re)  com- 
puted for  flow  around  a  polyp,  a  significant  inverse  rela- 
tionship is  found  (P  <  0.05;  df  =11),  and  the  efficiency 
is  remarkably  low  at  all  speeds  tested  for  feeding  in  dense 
concentrations  of  prey  (Fig.  3).  Re  =  Umeand/i',  where  d 
=  polyp  oral  disk  diameter,  and  v  =  kinematic  viscosity 
of  seawater.  Because  polyp  size  is  relatively  constant.  Re 
can  be  viewed  as  a  dimensionless  flow  speed. 

Discussion 

Mechanisms  of  particle  impact  ion 

Rubenstein  and  Koehl  (1977)  presented  dimensionless 
indices  for  various  mechanisms  of  particle  capture  by  sus- 


96 


M.  R.  PATTERSON 


A. 


flow 


S 


45 
40- 
35 
30 
25- 
20- 
15- 
JO- 
S' 
0 


3 


2 


4 


C. 


B. 


2 


Umean=2-7cm/s 


n  =  207 


1 


1 


I 


T 


5 


Umean=12-2cm/s 


o 

a. 


L 
x 


Hnean  =  19'8 


Upstream  Tentacles  — >  Downstream  Tentacles 

Figure  1.  Coordinate  system  used  to  quantify  prey  capture  in  individual  polyps  in  colonies  ofAliyonium. 
(A)  Quantification  in  the  circumferential  direction  around  the  polyp.  Note  the  pairing  of  tentacles  from  the 
bilaterally  symmetric  halves  of  the  polyp,  ;.t'..  prey  caught  on  tentacles  with  the  same  identification  number 
were  paired.  The  five  concentric  rings  delineate  the  dimensionless  length  coordinate  used  for  assessing  capture 
by  individual  tentacles.  (B)  The  distance  from  the  pharynx  toward  the  tentacle  tip  (X)  is  divided  by  the 
overall  length  of  the  tentacle  (L)  to  generate  the  dimensionless  distance  (X/L)  used  as  the  independent 
variable  (abscissa)  in  Figure  2.  (C)  Circumferential  position  of  prey  capture  events  in  individual  polyps  of 
Alcyonium  at  three  different  flow  speeds.  Data  from  the  bilateral  halves  of  the  colony  are  pooled;  see  Figure 
1 A  for  interpretation  of  abscissa.  Capture  frequencies  are  normalized  relative  to  the  amount  of  tentacle  area 
assigned  to  the  numbers  one  through  five.  Data  were  arc-sine  transformed  and  then  back-transformed  for 
graphical  portrayal.  Vertical  bars  are  95%  confidence  intervals.  For  flow  speeds  of  2.7,  12.2.  and  19.8  cm/ 
s,  the  total  numbers  of  cysts  caught  were  207,  205,  and  70.  respectively. 


pension  feeding  organisms.  Table  I  gives  these  values  for 
polyps  of  Alcyonium  feeding  on  Anemia  cysts.  Note  that 
direct  interception  or  "geometric"  interception  (Chang, 
1973)  has  the  highest  value  and  is  thus  most  likely  to  be 
the  dominant  mechanism  of  particle  capture.  For  a  rigid 
filter,  the  efficiency  of  capture  by  direct  interception 
should  be  independent  of  flow  speed  (Fuchs,  1964):  this 


is  contrary  to  the  pattern  observed.  A  formulation  of  the 
model  more  appropriate  to  aqueous  suspensions  invokes 
the  importance  of  short-range  (London- van  der  Waals) 
forces  and  does  not  ignore  the  hydrodynamic  resistance 
that  occurs  as  the  particle  squeezes  fluid  from  the  space 
between  itself  and  the  filter  element  (Chang,  1973;  La- 
Barbera,  1984).  This  more  refined  theory  predicts  an  in- 


POLYP-LEVEL  PREY  CAPTURE  EVENTS 


97 


Umean  =  2-7  cm/s 


LI 


60  • 
50  ' 
40  • 
1    30  ' 

u 
01 

ft,    20  • 
10  ' 

n  =  207 

==£±1 

1 

I 

1 

1 

mean 


=  12.2cm/s 


60 

n  =  205 

50  • 

T 

*-    40 

|    30" 

a* 

ft.    20  ' 

10  ' 

1 

0 

1 

=  19'8 


80  • 
70  ' 
60  ' 
£    50  ' 

01 

£    40  ' 
£    30  * 
20  ' 
10  ' 

n  =  70 

1 

1 

0               ' 

0.0     0.2     0.4     0.6     0.8     1.0 


0.0     0.2     0.4     0.6    0.8      1.0 


0.0    0.2     0.4     0.6     0.8     1.0 


Pharynx >  Tentacle  Tip  (X/L) 

Figure  2.  Location  of  prey  capture  events  along  the  length  of  tentacles  of  polyps  in  the  octocoral  Alcyonium 
at  three  different  flow  speeds.  See  Figure  I A  for  interpretation  of  abscissa.  Capture  frequencies  are  normali/ed 
relative  to  the  amount  of  tentacle  area  available  for  prey  capture.  Data  were  arc-sine  transformed  and  then 
back-transformed  for  graphical  portrayal.  Vertical  bars  are  95"i  confidence  intervals.  For  flow  speeds  of  2.7, 
12.2,  and  19.8  cm/s,  the  total  numbers  of  cysts  caught  were  207,  205,  and  70,  respectively. 


verse  relationship  between  flow  speed  at  the  surface  of 
the  filter  and  efficiency  of  particle  capture  and  is  here 
observed  in  Alcyonium  (Fig.  3).  Rubenstein  and  Koehl 
( 1977)  originally  predicted  that  direct  interception  would 
be  the  dominant  mode  of  feeding  in  aquatic  suspension 
feeders  and  subsequent  work  with  a  diversity  of  organisms 
seems  to  be  bearing  out  their  hypothesis  (LaBarbera, 
1984). 

Alcyonium  could  sieve  particles  larger  than  the  spacing 
between  pinnules  on  the  tentacles  (200-280  /urn)  as  sug- 
gested by  Sebens  and  Koehl  (1984).  However,  Anemia 
cysts  sieved  on  the  aboral  side  of  a  tentacle  were  almost 
always  dislodged  subsequently  by  the  flow  and  lost  to  the 
polyp  (pers.  obs.).  Anemia  cysts  were  occasionally  sieved 
by  tentacles  on  the  downstream  side  of  the  polyp.  For 
larger  prey  items,  sieving  may  be  an  important  capture 
mechanism,  but  probably  only  for  tentacles  on  the  down- 
stream side  of  a  polyp. 

The  parameter  for  diffusive  deposition  assumes  the 
particle  has  a  diffusion  coefficient  (D)  predicted  by 
Brownian  motion  for  a  particle  of  a  certain  size  in  a  given 
liquid  of  a  certain  temperature  and  viscosity.  If  eddy-en- 
hanced diffusion  is  allowed,  the  value  for  D  is  no  longer 
a  constant,  but  will  be  a  property  of  the  flow  speed  and 
eddy  size  (Richardson,  1926;  Okubo,  1971);  it  can  be  as 
much  as  104  larger  than  the  diffusion  coefficient  for  lam- 
inar or  "Fickian"  diffusion  (Okubo,  1980).  Turbulent  dif- 
fusive deposition  would  increase  by  a  similar  factor  and 
would  be  comparatively  more  important  as  the  size  of 
the  particle  of  interest  decreased,  perhaps  becoming  very 
important  as  a  mechanism  for  suspension  feeders  eating 


phyto-  and  bacterioplankton.  Schrij ver  el  ul.  (1981)  stud- 
ied particle  collection  efficiencies  by  small  glass  fibers  of 
about  the  same  diameter  as  the  tentacles  in  Alcyonium. 
For  small  particles  (ca.  5  Mm),  diffusive  deposition  was  a 
very  significant  mechanism.  More  work  is  needed  in  this 
area,  which  is  technically  difficult,  because  eddy  diffusiv- 
ities  near  surfaces  must  be  measured  (Denny,  1988),  and 


X 

w 

>•» 

(J 

.Si 
°3 

_ 

DC 
C 

"•5 

Ci 


100         200         300         400 
Polyp  Re 


500 


Figure  3.  Efficiency  (E)  of  particle  capture  per  polyp  in  colonies  of 
Alcyonium  as  a  function  of  polyp  Reynolds  number  (Re).  Efficiency  is 
denned  as  the  number  of  particles  caught  by  the  polyp  in  the  time  interval 
to  saturation  of  the  colony,  divided  by  the  number  of  particles  that  would 
have  passed  through  the  space  occupied  by  the  polyp.  Reynolds  number 
was  calculated  using  the  oral  disk  diameter  of  the  polyp  and  flow  speed 
measured  1.0  cm  above  the  oral  disk.  Whole  colony  Re  for  the  specimens 
used  was  10-40  x  greater  than  polyp  Re.  The  regression  is  given  by  the 
equation:  E  =  2066  Re'1 55;  P  <  0.05;  r  =  0.92;  df  =  11. 


98 


M.  R.  PATTERSON 


the  convection  of  unfiltered  water  to  the  vicinity  of  the 
filter  should  be  considered.  Methods  of  carefully  releasing 
dye  and  studying  its  motion  near  filter  feeders  using  image 
processing  techniques  are  being  developed  to  allow  better 
investigation  of  microscale  turbulent  diffusive  deposition 
(unpub.  obs.). 

Rates  of  particle  encounter  and  possible  capture  by 
gravitational  deposition  are  independent  of  flow  speed 
but  directly  proportional  to  settling  velocity.  The  settling 
velocity  for  Anemia  cysts  is  absolutely  low,  so  the  total 
flux  of  particles  to  the  polyps  via  this  mechanism  is  much 
lower  than  the  contribution  provided  by  direct  intercep- 
tion. It  is  unlikely  that  natural  food  particles  have  settling 
velocities  appreciably  greater  than  Anemia  cysts.  The 
contribution  of  inertial  impaction  to  the  capture  of  par- 
ticles at  higher  Reynolds  number  could  be  a  potentially 
important  mechanism  if  particle  inertia  is  appreciable. 
The  upstream  side  of  an  individual  tentacle  will  have  a 
stagnation  point  and  the  flow  will  split  at  this  point  (Fig. 
4)  and  flow  around  the  tentacle  at  low  flow  speeds  or  up 
and  over  the  tentacular  crown  at  higher  Re  (Patterson, 
1984).  A  calculation  in  the  Appendix  demonstrates  that 
this  mechanism  is  highly  unlikely  to  be  an  important 
mode  of  particle  capture  by  polyps  in  this  octocoral. 

Feeding  efficiency  of  cnidarian  filters 

The  filtration  efficiency  as  calculated  per  Dorman 
( 1 966)  is  very  low  for  individual  polyps  (Fig.  3);  a  possible 
explanation  involves  partitioning  capture  efficiency  into 
collection  efficiency  and  adhesion  efficiency  (Weber  et  ai. 
1983).  Many  particles  that  appear  to  strike  the  surface  of 


Figure  4.  Streamlines  of  fluid  How  near  the  upstream  stagnation 
point  of  a  tiller  (Bird  el  al..  1960),  where  inertial  impaction  is  most  likely 
to  occur.  X  and  Y  are  directional  coordinates.  The  upstream  plane  (in- 
dicated by  X0)  marks  where  particles  earned  by  the  flow  have  not  yet 
deviated  from  the  streamlines  due  to  their  inertia.  See  the  Appendix  for 
a  derivation  showing  how  inertial  impaction  is  not  likely  to  be  an  im- 
portant capture  mechanism  for  cnidanans  that  use  passive  suspension 
feeding. 


A/croniiim  tentacles  are  not  trapped;  relatively  few  are 
lost  if  they  initially  adhere,  although  J.  Miles  (Northeastern 
Univ.  pers.  comm.)  found  that  adhesion  and  loss  was  sig- 
nificantly affected  by  flow  speed  in  the  sea  anemone,  Me- 
tridium  senile,  and  Leonard  et  al.  (1988)  found  that  flow 
speed  affected  capture  probabilities  in  a  crinoid.  My  study 
of  capture  at  the  level  of  the  polyp  addresses  successful 
prey  capture  events  only. 

Future  work  on  the  mechanisms  of  particle  capture  by 
cnidarians  should  investigate  the  role  of  unsuccessful 
adhesions.  In  particular,  the  importance  of  London-van 
der  Waals  forces  versus  nematocyst  firing  should  be  ex- 


Table  I 

\ 'allies  of  ditnensionless  parameters  for  four  potential  modes  oj  particle  capture  (cf.  Rubenstein  and  Koehl.  1977)  in  the  octocoral  Alcyonium 
siderium  feeding  on  Artemia  cysts  al  two  di/lerenl  flow  speeds  commonly  encountered  in  nature 


Gravitational 
deposition 

Direct 
interception 

Inertial  impaction 

Diffusive 
deposition 

«"         II 
L1!) 

di       . 

1  8  ^df 

dfLJo 

U0  =  3.0  cm/s                               0.027 
U0  =  30.0  cm/s                             0.003 

0.67 
0.67 

4.0  x  10"6 
4.0  x  10~s 

8.2  X  10"" 
8.2  X  10~12 

dp  =  particle  diameter  =  0.02  cm  (Patterson,  1984). 

dr  =  filter  element  diameter  =  0.03  cm  (Sebens  and  Koehl,  1984). 

Ug  =  Stokes"  settling  velocity  =  0.08  cm/s  (Gibbs,  1985). 

PP  =  density  of  particle  =  1.05  g/ml  (Gibbs.  1985). 

pm  =  density  of  seawater  =  1.02  g/ml  at  10°C  (Zerbe  and  Taylor.  1953). 

>i  =  dynamic  viscosity  of  seawater  =  1.4  X  10~2  g/(cm  -s)  at  IO°C  (Sverdrup  el  a!.,  1942). 

U0  =  flow  near  the  tentacle  (two  values  used  in  table  above;  cf.  Patterson,  1984;  Patterson  and  Sebens,  1989). 

K.T 

D  =  diffusion  coefficient  of  an  Artemia  cyst  =  =  7.4  X  10~12  cm2/s. 

6!T^dp 

KT  =  energy  of  thermal  fluctuation  =  3.9  x  1C)-'4  (g-cm2)/s2. 


POLYP-LEVEL  PREY  CAPTURE  EVENTS 


99 


plored,  as  current  data  are  insufficient  to  address  this  issue. 
The  subject  bears  further  attention  because  Best  (1988) 
also  found  that  feeding  efficiency  was  an  inverse  function 
of  flow  speed  (read  Reynolds  number)  in  the  sea  pen, 
Ptilosarcus gumeyi.  She  attributed  this  decline  to  the  pre- 
dicted behavior  of  particles  that  are  caught  by  direct  in- 
terception (Spielman,  1977),  perhaps  aided  by  a  defor- 
mation of  the  filter  elements  as  the  hydrodynamic  drag 
increased.  Alcyonium  polyps  on  the  upstream  side  of  col- 
onies deform  in  strong  flows  while  polyps  in  the  wake 
undergo  little  deformation  (Patterson,  1984).  The  octo- 
corals  she  studied  are  more  efficient  than  Alcyonium  by 
a  factor  of  five  at  a  similar  flow  speed.  This  study  used 
only  polyps  located  near  the  top  of  the  colony.  Increasing 
mechanical  deformation  of  this  subpopulation  of  polyps 
occurred  at  higher  flows,  resulting  in  less  surface  area 
available  for  prey  capture,  but  the  reduction  was  detected 
in  the  measurements  of  projected  colony  surface  area. 
The  reasons  for  this  interspecific  difference  in  feeding  ef- 
fectiveness are  presently  unknown. 

Panicle  capture  locations 

The  patterns  of  prey  capture  seen  (Figs.  1C,  2)  are  in 
agreement  with  a  direct  interception  model  of  prey  cap- 
ture. Polyps  resemble  inverted  umbrellas  (see  photograph 
in  Sebens  and  Koehl,  1984);  they  do  not  hold  their  ten- 
tacles in  a  flat  canopy  as  the  top  view  in  Figure  1 A  might 
imply.  At  low  speeds,  the  first  tentacles  to  encounter  par- 
ticles are  the  upstream  ones,  and  here  capture  is  more 
likely.  As  the  flow  speed  increases,  the  polyps  are  bent  by 
the  flow  (cf.  Patterson,  1984).  and  the  aboral  side  of  the 
upstream  polyps  is  presented  to  the  flow.  For  prey  particles 
the  size  of  Anemia  cysts  (ca.  200  ^m;  close  in  value  to 
the  mean  size  of  natural  zooplankton  prey,  Sebens  and 
Koehl,  1984),  few  particles  are  caught  on  these  upstream 
tentacles  because  particles  do  not  adhere  to  the  aboral 
side.  The  downstream  tentacles  then  begin  feeding.  In 
strong  flows,  the  polyp  is  severely  bent  downstream,  and 
a  small  eddy  forms  over  the  tentacular  disk  (pers.  obs.); 
the  distribution  becomes  roughly  symmetric  again.  The 
relative  roles  that  attached  eddy  formulation  and  turbulent 
diffusion  play  in  this  smoothing  out  of  the  particle  capture 
distribution  are  unknown. 

These  experiments  were  conducted  under  turbulent  but 
steady  flow  conditions.  Alcyonium  occurs  over  a  range  of 
depths  and  habitats  in  the  shallow  subtidal;  it  is  exposed 
to  both  oscillatory  flow  from  wind-driven  waves  and  to 
steady  tidal  flows  (Patterson,  1984,  1985).  The  particle 
capture  behavior  of  individual  polyps  and  tentacles  might 
be  quite  different  in  an  oscillating  flow.  Hunter  (1989) 
found  that  the  feeding  effectiveness  of  the  hydroid  Obelia 
longissima  was  much  greater  in  an  oscillating  flow  relative 
to  a  steady  current.  Alcyonium  colonies  are  inherently 


more  rigid  than  those  of  Ohelia.  and  hence  it  is  not  clear 
without  further  experimentation  whether  oscillatory  flow 
would  result  in  enhanced  feeding  in  Alcyonium.  Thus 
these  results  should  be  applied  only  to  feeding  in  the  field 
under  steady  flow  conditions  when  wind-driven  oscilla- 
tions have  a  small  contribution  to  the  flow  field. 

The  distribution  of  particle  capture  over  the  length  of 
an  individual  tentacle  is  also  in  agreement  with  a  geo- 
metric (direct)  interception  model.  Parts  of  the  tentacle 
furthest  out  of  the  boundary  layer  of  the  polyp  intercept 
the  most  prey.  Patterns  of  prey  capture  discerned  through 
flume  experiments  using  non-motile  particles  such  asAr- 
lemia  cysts  may  be  different  from  those  measured  using 
live  prey,  but  only  if  the  loss  rate  of  captured  particles 
differs  between  the  two  types  of  food  or  the  motility  of 
live  prey  causes  the  diffusive  deposition  mechanism  to 
increase  capture  preferentially  at  a  location  different  from 
direct  interception.  Loss  of  captured  particles  is  most  likely 
caused  by  hydrodynamic  drag  forces  exceeding  the  break- 
ing strength  of  the  attachment  between  particle  and  ten- 
tacle. Live  zooplankton  prey  and  Anemia  cysts  will  ex- 
perience very  similar  amounts  of  drag  because  size  and 
shape  are  similar.  The  motility  of  live  zooplankton  should 
result  in  an  increase  in  the  diffusive  particle  flux  relative 
to  Anemia  cysts,  but  it  should  not  affect  the  geometric 
location  of  capture  on  tentacles  if  movement  is  random 
in  all  directions. 

This  study  has  shown  how  flow  regime  can  dramatically 
affect  patterns  of  particle  capture  at  the  level  of  the  filtering 
elements  in  an  octocoral.  Variation  in  feeding  ability  at 
the  level  of  the  polyp  caused  by  hydrodynamics  may  help 
explain  the  variation  Lasker  (1981)  observed  in  prey  cap- 
ture between  polyps  and  branches  in  colonies  of  tropical 
gorgonians.  Capture  events  in  the  three  species  Lasker 
studied  did  not  fit  a  Poisson  distribution,  and  he  invoked 
differential  feeding  ability  of  the  polyps  as  the  cause  of 
the  variation.  He  offered  no  explanation  for  the  differential 
feeding  ability  other  than  to  note  that  other  authors  had 
also  seen  asymmetric  patterns  in  prey  capture  by  cnidar- 
ians  (e.g.,  Leversee,  1976).  I  have  demonstrated  that  mo- 
mentum transport  (fluid  flow)  directly  affects  mass  trans- 
port (particle  capture)  at  the  level  of  the  individual  feeding 
elements,  polyps.  Upon  closer  inspection,  other  passive 
suspension  feeding  cnidarians  may  exhibit  similar  pat- 
terns. 

Acknowledgments 

This  paper  has  benefited  from  discussions  with  P.  Bas- 
ser,  T.  Givnish,  M.  Koehl,  M.  LaBarbera,  T.  McMahon, 
R.  Olson,  S.  L.  Sanderson,  K.  Sebens,  and  R.  Turner.  T. 
McMahon  provided  inspiration  and  insight  into  the  me- 
chanics of  particle  deposition  through  his  graduate  course 
on  fluid  flow  in  the  human  body.  P.  Rudy,  Acting  Di- 


100 


M.  R.  PATTERSON 


rector.  Marine  Science  Center,  Northeastern  University, 
Nahant,  Massachusetts,  kindly  provided  laboratory  space. 
C.  Alexander  provided  technical  assistance.  Financial 
support  for  this  project  was  provided  by  the  Richmond 
Fund  of  Harvard  University,  the  Lerner-Gray  Fund  for 
Marine  Research  of  the  American  Museum  of  Natural 
History,  a  Faculty  Research  Award  from  UC  Davis,  and 
NSFOCE87- 16427. 

Literature  Cited 

Bell,  K.  1974.  Aerosol  deposition  in  models  of  a  human  lung  bifur- 
cation. Ph.D.  Dissertation.  California  Institute  of  Technology.  Pas- 
adena. 

Berg,  H.  C.  1983.  Random  Walks  in  Biology.  Princeton  University 
Press.  Princeton. 

Best,  B.  A.  1988.  Passive  suspension  feeding  in  a  sea  pen:  effects  of 
ambient  flow  on  volume  flow  rate  and  filtering  efficiency.  Dial.  Bull 
175:  332-342. 

Bird,  R.  B.,  VV.  E.  Stewart,  and  E.  N.  Lightfoot.  I960.  Transport  Phe- 
nomena. John  Wiley.  New  York. 

Brodkey,  R.  S.,  and  H.  C.  Hershey.  1988.  Transport  Phenomena:  A 
Unified  Approach  McGraw-Hill,  New  York. 

Chang,  D.  P.  Y.  1973.  Particle  collection  from  aqueous  suspensions 
by  solid  and  hollow  single  fibers.  Ph.D.  Dissertation.  California  In- 
stitute of  Technology.  Pasadena. 

Denny,  M.  1988.  Biology  and  the  Mechanics  of  the  Ware-Swept  En- 
vironment. Princeton  University  Press,  Princeton. 

Dorman,  R.  G.  1966.  Filtration.  Pp.  195-222  in  Aerosol  Science. 
C.  N.  Davies,  ed.  Academic  Press.  New  York. 

Kindeisen,  VV.  1935.  Uber  das  Abstetzen  kleiner.  in  der  Luft  suspen- 
dierter  Teilchen  in  der  menschlichen  Lunge  bei  der  Atmung.  PJhiger 
Arch.  6>.v.  Physiol.  236:  367-379. 

Fuchs,  N.  A.  1964.  The  Mechanics  of  Aerosols.  Pergamon  Press.  New 
York. 

Gibbs,  R.  J.  1985.  Estuarine  floes:  their  size,  settling  velocity,  and  den- 
sity. /  Geophys.  Res  90(C2):  3249-3251. 

Glauert,  M.  1940.  A  method  of  constructing  the  paths  of  raindrops  of 
different  diameters  moving  in  the  neighbourhood  of  (Da  circular 
cylinder.  (2)  an  aerofoil,  placed  in  a  uniform  stream  of  air:  and  a 
determination  of  the  rate  of  deposit  of  the  drops  on  the  surface  and 
the  percentage  of  drops  caught.  Aeronaut.  Res  Council.  Reports  and 
Memoranda  No.  2025.  London. 

Harvell,  D.,  and  M.  LaBarbera.  1985.  Flexibility:  a  mechanism  for 
control  of  local  velocity  in  hydroid  colonies.  Biol.  Bull  168:  312- 
320. 

Holland,  N.  D.,  A.  B.  Leonard,  and  J.  R.  Strickler.  1987.  Upstream 
and  downstream  capture  during  suspension  feeding  by  Oligometra 
serripinna  (Echinodermata:  Crinoideal  under  surge  conditions.  Biol. 
Bull.  173:  552-556. 

Hunter,  T.  1989.  Suspension  feeding  in  oscillating  flow:  the  effect  of 
colony  morphology  and  flow  regime  on  plankton  capture  by  the  hy- 
droid  Obelia  longissima.  Biol.  Bull.  176:  41-49. 

Koehl,  M.  A.  R.,  and  J.  R.  Strickler.  1981.  Copepod  feeding  currents: 
food  capture  at  low  Reynolds  number.  Limnol.  Oceanogr  26:  1062- 
1093. 

LaBarbera,  M.  1978.  Particle  capture  by  a  Pacific  brittle  star:  experi- 
mental test  of  the  aerosol  suspension  feeding  model.  Science  201: 
1147-1149. 

LaBarbera,  M.  1984.  Feeding  currents  and  particle  capture  mechanisms 
in  suspension  feeding  animals.  Am.  Zool.  24:  71-84. 

LaBarbera,  M.,  and  S.  Vogel.  1976.  An  inexpensive  thermistor  flow- 
meter  for  aquatic  biology.  Limnol.  Oceanogr.  21:  750-756. 


Landahl,  H.  D.  1950.     On  the  removal  of  air-borne  droplets  by  the 

human  respiratory  tract:  1.  The  lung.  Bull.  Math.  Biophys.  12:  43- 

56. 
Landahl,  H.  D.  1963.     Particle  removal  by  the  respiratory  system:  note 

on  the  removal  of  airborne  particulates  by  the  human  respiratory 

tract  with  particular  reference  to  the  role  of  diffusion.  A  M.  A.  Arch. 

Ind.  Hyg  Occup.  Med.  25:  29-39. 
Lasker,  H.  R.  1981.     A  comparison  of  the  paniculate  feeding  abilities 

of  three  species  of  gorgonian  soft  coral.  Mar.  Ecol.  Prog.  Ser.  5:  61- 

67. 
Leversee,  G.  J.  1976.     Flow  and  feeding  in  fan-shaped  colonies  of  a 

gorgonian  coral.  Leptogorgia.  Biol.  Bull.  151:  344-356. 
Leonard,  A.  B.,  J.  R.  Strickler,  and  N.  D.  Holland.  1988.     Effects  of 

current  speed  on  filtration  during  suspension  feeding  in  Oligometra 

serripinna  (Echinodermata:  Crinoidea).  Mar.  Biol  97:  1 1 1-122. 
Lewis,  J.  B.,  and  \V.  S.  Price.  1976.     Patterns  of  ciliary  currents  in 

Atlantic  reef  corals  and  their  functional  significance.  J.  Zool  178: 

77-89. 

McMahon,  T.  A.,  J.  D.  Brain,  and  S.  Lemott.  1977.     Species  differ- 
ences in  aerosol  deposition.  Pp.  23-33  in  Inhaled  Particles  IT. 

W.  H.  Walton,  ed.  Pergamon  Press.  Oxford. 
Merz,  R.  1984.     Self-generated  versus  environmentally  produced  feeding 

currents:  a  comparison  for  the  Sabellid  polychaete  Eudislylia  van- 

couven  Biol.  Bull.  167:  200-209. 
Niklas,  K.  J.  1982a.     Pollination  and  airflow  patterns  around  conifer 

ovulate  cones.  Science  217:  442-444. 
Niklas,  K.  J.  1982b.     Simulated  and  empiric  wind  pollination  patterns 

of  conifer  ovulate  cones.  Proc.  Nat.  Acad.  Sci.  USA  79:  510-514. 
Okamura,  B.  1984.     The  effects  of  ambient  flow  velocity,  colony  size. 

and  upstream  colonies  on  the  feeding  success  of  Bryozoa..  I.  Bugida 

stolonijera  Ryland.  an  arborescent  species.  J.  Exp.  Mar.  Biol.  Ecol. 

83:  179-193. 
Okamura,  B.  1987.     Particle  size  and  flow  velocity  induce  an  inferred 

switch  in  bryozoan  suspension-feeding  behavior.  Biol.  Bull.  173:  222- 

229. 
Okubo,  A.  1971.  Oceanic  diffusion  diagrams.  Deep-Sea  Res.  18:  789- 

802. 
Okubo,  A.  1980.     Di/fusion  ami  Ecological  Problems:  Mathematical 

Models.  (Biomathematics,  Vol.  10).  Springer  Verlag.  New  York. 
Patterson,  M.  R.  1980.     Hydromechanical  adaptations  in  Alcyonium 

.sulerium  (Octocorallia).  Pp.  183-201  in  Biofluid  Mechanics  2.  D.  J. 

Schneck,  ed.  Plenum,  New  York. 
Patterson,  M.  R.  1984.     Patterns  of  whole  colony  prey  capture  in  the 

octocoral.  Alcyonium  siderium.  Biol.  Bull.  167:  613-629. 
Patterson,  M.  R.  1985.     The  effects  of  flow  on  the  biology  of  passive 

suspension  feeders:  prey  capture,  feeding  rate,  and  gas  exchange  in 

selected  cnidarians.  Ph.D.  dissertation.  Harvard  University.  Cam- 
bridge. Massachusetts. 
Patterson,  M.  R.,  and  K.  P.  Sebens.  1989.     Forced  convection  modulates 

gas  exchange  in  cnidarians.  Proc.  Nat.  Acad.  Sci.  USA  86:  8833- 

8836. 
Patterson,  M.  R.  1991.     Passive  suspension  feeding  by  an  octocoral  in 

plankton  patches:  empirical  test  of  a  mathematical  model.  Biol.  Bull 

180:81-92. 

Richardson,  L.  F.  1926.     Atmospheric  diffusion  shown  on  a  distance- 
neighbor  graph.  Proc.  Roy.  Soc.  London  A  110:  709-727. 
Ross,  D.  H.,  and  D.  A.  Craig.  1980.     Mechanisms  of  fine  particle  capture 

by  larval  black  flies  (Diptera:  Simuliidae).  Can  J  Zool.  58:  1 186- 

1192. 
Rubenstein,  D.  J.,  and  M.  A.  R.  Koehl.  1977.     The  mechanism  of  filter 

feeding:  some  theoretical  considerations.  Am.  Nat.  Ill:  981-994. 
Schrijver,  J.  H.  M.,  C.  Vreeker,  and  J.  A.  Wesselingh.  1981 .     Deposition 

of  particles  on  a  cylindrical  collector.  J.  Coll.  Inter/.  Sci.  81:  249- 

256. 


POLYP-LEVEL  PREY  CAPTURE  EVENTS 


101 


Sebens,  K.  P.  1984.     Water  flow  and  coral  colony  size:  mterhahilal 

comparisons  of  the  octocoral  Alcyoniwn  sidenum  Proc.  Nat.  Acad. 

Sci  (i'SA)Sl:  5473-5477. 
Sebens,  K.  P.  1986.     Spatial  relationships  among  encrusting  marine 

organisms  in  the  New  England  subtidal  zone.  Ecu!.  Monogr.  56:  73- 

96. 
Sebens,  K.  P.,  and  M.  A.  R.  Koehl.  1984.     Predation  on  zooplankton 

by  the  benthic  anthozoans.  Alcyoniwn  sideriuni  (Alcyonacea)  and 

Metridium  senile  (Actiniaria)  in  the  New  England  subtidal.  Mar 

Biol  81:  255-271. 
Spielman.  I..  A.  1977.     Particle  capture  from  low-speed  laminar  Hows. 

Anna  Re\   l-'liiu/ Mec/L  9:  297-319. 
Strathmann,  R.  R.,  and  E.  Liese.  1979.     On  feeding  mechanisms  and 

clearance  rates  of  molluscan  veligers.  Biol.  Bull-  157:  524-535. 
Sverdrup,  H.  U.,  M.  W.  Johnson,  and  R.  H.  Fleming.  1942.     The  Oceans: 

Their  Physics,  Chemistry,  and  Biology  Prentice-Hall,  New  York. 
Taghon,  G.  I,.,  A.  R.  M.  Nowell,  and  P.  A.  Jumars.  1980.     Induction 

of  suspension  feeding  in  spionid  polychaetes  by  high  particle  fluxes. 

Science  210:  562-564. 
Taylor,  C.  I.  1940.     Notes  on  possible  equipment  and  technique  for 

experiments  on  icing  on  aircraft.  Aeronaut   Res   Council.  Report  \ 

and  Memoranda  No.  2024.  London. 
Vogel,  S.,  and  M.  LaBarbera.  1978.     Simple  flow  tanks  for  research 

and  teaching.  BioScience  10:  638-643. 

Weber,  M.  E.,  D.  C.  Blanchard,  and  L.  D.  Syzdek.  1983.     The  mech- 
anism ot  scavenging  of  waterborne  bacteria  by  a  rising  bubble,  l.tmnol 

Oceanogr.  28(1):  101-105. 
Zerbe,  VV.  B.,  and  C.  B.  Taylor.  1953.     Sea  water  density  reduction 

tables.  Pp.  18-19  in  Coast  and  Geodetic  Survey.  Special  f'uhl  no 

298.  U.  S.  Dept.  of  Commerce,  Washington.  DC. 


Appendix 

The  following  derivation,  developed  from  the  work  of 
Glauert  (1940)  on  raindrop  capture  by  airfoils  and  from 
Taylor  ( 1940)  on  aircraft  icing,  shows  how  unlikely  it  is 
that  inertial  impaction  will  be  an  important  mechanism 
for  passive  suspension  feeding  cnidarians  for  the  range  of 
velocities  normally  encountered  in  the  field. 

Consider  the  motion  of  a  solid  particle  (e.g..  a  plankter), 
moving  with  a  velocity  relative  to  the  seawater,  as  it  ap- 
proaches the  upstream  end  of  a  filtering  organism  (Fig. 
4).  In  other  words,  the  particle  does  not  follow  the  stream- 
lines perfectly.  If  the  Reynolds  number  (Re)  of  the  particle 
is  on  the  order  of  one  or  less,  as  it  would  be  for  plankton- 
size  particles  (Koehl  and  Strickler,  1 98 1 ),  the  forces  acting 
on  the  particle  are  due  solely  to  the  Stokes'  drag  (Berg, 
1983).  The  equation  governing  the  motion  of  the  particle 
in  the  x-direction  is: 


3 


dU 


dt 


p  _ 


=  67rrM(U  -  UD) 


(Eq.  1) 


where  r  =  particle  radius.  ps  =  particle  density,  n  =  dy- 
namic viscosity,  Up  =  velocity  component  of  the  particle 
in  the  x-direction,  and  U  =  velocity  component  of  the 
seawater  in  the  x-direction. 


The  velocity  components  in  the  y-  and  z-directions  are 
V  and  W,  respectively,  and  the  equations  of  motion  are 
similar.  If  I  define: 


(Eq.2) 


then  (Eq.  1)  becomes: 


ko-^  -Up  (Eq.3) 

and  other  directions  can  be  transformed  similarly. 

The  equations  of  motion  must  be  solved  subject  to  the 
initial  conditions.  Let  me  introduce  a  scaled  time  variable. 


and  the  differential  operator, 

£  =  l 

Now  (Eq.  3)  becomes 
dUn 


dt 


(Eq.4) 


(Eq.5) 


(Eq.6) 


and  similarly  for  the  other  directions. 

Flow  near  the  upstream  stagnation  point  of  the  filter 
will  look  like  Figure  4  (Bird  c/  a/..  1960).  The  velocity 
field  near  the  stagnation  point  is  given  by: 


U  =  -ex 


and 


V  =  cy 
Substituting  into  (Eq.  6),  I  obtain: 

d:x       dx 

^3  +  -  +  cM  = 


(Eq.7) 


(Eq.8) 


(Eq.9) 


It  is  reasonable  to  assume  that  upstream  of  the  tentacle 
a  certain  distance,  X0,  the  flow  field  is  not  distorted  by 
the  presence  of  the  tentacle  and  the  particle  is  following 
the  streamlines  of  the  moving  seawater  (see  Fig.  4).  If  the 
time  at  which  the  particle  starts  to  deviate  from  the 
streamlines  of  flow  is  called  T  =  0,  I  obtain: 


x(0)  =  -  X0 
dt 


(Eq. 10) 
(Eq.ll) 


A  reasonable  guess  to  the  solution  of  (Eq.  1 1 )  is  one  of 
the  form  x(t)  =  Aepl,  where  A  =  constant.  The  charac- 
teristic equation  is  thus. 


p:  +  p  + 


=  0 


(Eq.12) 


102 


M.   R.  PATTERSON 


The  roots  of  the  equation  are: 


PL:  = 


- 1  ±  VI  - 


(Eq. 13) 


When  one  of  the  roots  is  imaginary,  the  solution  to  the 
equation  of  motion  of  the  passing  particle  will  be  oscil- 
latory, i.e..  the  particle  position  and  the  filter  position  will 
eventually  coincide  (x  =  0).  This  condition  will  occur 
when: 


4ck<, 


(Eq. 14) 


Glauert  (1940)  showed  for  a  cylindrical  geometry  that 
a  negligible  number  of  particles  will  impact  if  k^c^s  0.125, 
where  c  =  2  and  the  definition  of  ko  is  as  follows: 


2psrRU 
9pR2v 


(Eq. 15) 


where  R  =  radius  of  the  filter,  and  v  =  n/p,  the  kinematic 
viscosity  of  the  seawater. 

Using  reasonable  values  for  flow  around  an  Alcyonium 
colony,  I  obtain  k<jC  =  0.003,  for  p  =  1.024  g/cm\  (Zerbe 
and  Taylor,  1953),  p,  =  1.049  g/cm3,  (Gibbs,  1985),  r 
=  100  X  10~4cm,  R  =  5cm,  U  =  5  cm/s,  v=  1.36X  10~: 
cnr/s  for  seawater  at  10°C  (calculated  from  Sverdrup 
el  til..  1942).  If  the  flow  speed  increases  by  an  order  of  mag- 
nitude to  U  =  50  cm/s,  then  koC  =  0.034.  Appreciable  im- 
paction  will  not  occur  until  U  =  185  cm/s,  far  above  the 
range  of  speeds  normally  encountered  near  this  species  (Pat- 
terson and  Sebens,  1989).  Such  a  flow  would  only  be  found 
under  stormy  conditions  in  the  subtidal  or  in  tidal  currents 
in  fjords.  Alcyonium  contracts  its  prey-capturing  surfaces 
long  before  this  flow  speed  is  obtained  (Patterson.  1980). 


Reference:  Biol.  Bull  180:  103-1 1 1.  (February,  1991) 


Differential  Ingestion  and  Digestion  of  Bivalve  Larvae 

by  the  Scyphozoan  Chrysaora  quinquedrrha 

and  the  Ctenophore  Mnemiopsis  leidyi 

JENNIFER  E.  PURCELL1.  FRANCES  P.  CRESSWELL1.  DAVID  G.  CARGO2, 

AND  VICTOR  S.  KENNEDY1 

The  University  of  Maryland 


Abstract.  We  investigated  predation  on  bivalve  veligers 
by  the  scyphozoan  Chrysaora  quinquedrrha  and  the 
Ctenophore  Mnemiopsis  leidyi.  We  found  that  the  medusa 
stage  of  C.  quinquedrrha  captures,  but  does  not  digest, 
veliger  larvae:  99%  of  oyster  veligers  (Crassostrea  virgin- 
tea)  caught  by  medusae  were  egested  alive  within  7  h  of 
capture,  and  98%  survived  for  24  h  after  egestion;  98%  of 
oyster,  mussel  (Afylilus  edulis),  and  clam  (Mulinia  later- 
alis)  veligers  placed  on  the  oral  arms  of  medusae  were 
rejected;  all  bivalve  veligers  in  field-collected  medusae 
were  closed  and  full  of  tissue.  Our  laboratory  evidence 
suggests  that  the  shell  of  larval  bivalves  probably  offers 
protection  from  medusae:  23%)  of  dead,  open  veligers  were 
ingested  by  medusae  compared  with  0.7%  of  live,  closed 
veligers;  open  veligers  were  retained  longer  than  closed 
veligers;  and  tissue  excised  from  recently  settled  oyster 
larvae  was  ingested  and  digested.  Freeswimming  C  quin- 
quedrrha ephyrae  ingested  but  did  not  digest  veligers.  By 
contrast,  the  benthic  scyphistoma  stage  ingested  69%  of 
veligers  that  contacted  their  tentacles  and  digested  48% 
of  those  ingested.  Each  scyphistoma  consumed  an  average 
of  1  veliger/day  at  densities  of  0.3  veligers  ml" ' .  However, 
larval  settlement  was  not  reduced  on  oyster  shells  bearing 
scyphistomae.  By  contrast  to  the  results  on  C.  quinque- 
drrha, ctenophores  egested  only  4%  of  veligers  alive,  and 
25%  of  the  veligers  in  their  gut  contents  were  digested. 
Predation  on  veligers  by  ctenophores  was  estimated  to  be 
0.2  to  1.7%/day  in  Chesapeake  Bay.  We  conclude  that  C. 


Received  14  August  1990;  accepted  6  November  1990. 

1  Horn  Point  Environmental  Laboratories,  P.  O.  Box  775,  Cambridge, 
Maryland  2 161 3. 

2  Chesapeake  Biological  Laboratory,  Box  38,  Solomons,  Maryland 

20688-0038. 


quinquedrrha  medusae  are  not  important  predators  of 
bivalve  veligers,  but  rather  may  reduce  their  mortality  by 
consuming  ctenophores,  which  do  eat  veligers. 

Introduction 

Predation  on  planktonic  larvae  is  one  of  the  least  un- 
derstood factors  affecting  abundance  of  adult  benthic  in- 
vertebrates! YoungandChia,  1987).  Early  studies  reported 
that  the  scyphomedusan  Chrysaora  quinquedrrha  (DeSor) 
and  the  Ctenophore  Mnemiopsis  leidyi  A.  Agassiz  may 
prey  heavily  upon  the  larvae  of  the  eastern  oyster  Cras- 
sostrea virginica  (Gmelin)  (Truitt  and  Mook,  1925;  and 
Nelson,  1925,  1953,  respectively).  Both  species  are  sea- 
sonally abundant  in  Atlantic  coast  estuaries,  and  co-occur 
with  oyster  larvae.  Their  effects  on  survival  of  oyster  larvae 
have  not  been  documented. 

In  several  Atlantic  coast  estuaries,  M.  leidyi  has  been 
shown  to  be  an  important  predator  of  crustacean  zoo- 
plankton  (e.g..  Cronin  et  ai.  1962;  Cargo  and  Schultz, 
1967;  Bishop,  1967;  Burrell,  1968;  Herman  et  ai.  1968; 
Kremer,  1979;  Deason  and  Smayda,  1982;  Feigenbaum 
and  Kelly,  1984;  Olson,  1987)  and  bivalve  veliger  larvae 
(Nelson,  1925;  Truitt  and  Mook,  1925;  Burrell  and  Van 
Engel,  1976).  Bivalve  veligers  were  75%  of  the  prey  of  M. 
leidyi  in  New  Jersey  waters,  and  high  larval  settlement  of 
three  bivalve  species,  including  oysters,  occurred  in  years 
when  Ctenophore  densities  were  low  (Nelson,  1925).  In 
the  York  River,  Virginia,  bivalve  larvae  were  inversely 
related  to  the  biomass  of  ctenophores  (Burrell  and  Van 
Engel,  1976). 

Studies  on  the  feeding  of  scyphomedusae  have  shown 
them  to  eat  a  variety  of  zooplankton  (reviewed  in  Larson, 
1978;  Clifford  and  Cargo,  1978;  Feigenbaum  and  Kelly, 
1984;  Larson,  1987;  Fancett,  1988;  Brewer,  1989).  Al- 


103 


104 


J.  E.  PURCELL  ET  AL 


though  C.  quinquecirrha  medusae  were  reported  to  feed 
on  oyster  larvae  (Truitt  and  Mook,  1925;  Loosanoff, 
1974).  high  numbers  of  oyster  larvae  and  medusae  often 
co-occurred  (Truitt  and  Mook,  1925).  This  apparent  par- 
adox may  be  due  to  the  fact  that  C.  quinquecirrha  medusae 
prey  heavily  upon  ctenophores  (Cargo  and  Schultz,  1967; 
Burrell,  1968;  Miller,  1974;  Feigenbaum  and  Kelly.  1984; 
Larson,  1986),  thus  decreasing  ctenophore  predation  on 
oyster  larvae. 

Nothing  is  known  of  the  trophic  ecology  of  the  incon- 
spicuous benthic  scyphistoma  or  early  free-swimming 
ephyra  stages  of  scyphozoans.  Large  numbers  of  C.  quin- 
quecirrha scyphistomae  are  found  on  oyster  shell  (Cargo 
and  Schultz,  1966,  1967),  which  is  a  preferred  settling 
substrate  for  oyster  larvae  (Kennedy  and  Breisch,  1981 ). 
Therefore,  these  scyphistomae  may  be  predators  of  oyster 
pediveliger  larvae  that  are  preparing  to  settle  upon  oyster 
shells. 

To  test  the  potential  importance  of  C.  quinquecirrha 
and  M.  leidyi  as  predators  of  bivalve  larvae,  we  compare 

( 1 )  medusa  and  ctenophore  digestion  of  oyster  veligers. 

(2)  rejection  or  ingestion  of  oyster,  blue  mussel  (Alytilus 
edulis  L.),  and  coot  clam  [Mulinia  lateralis  (Say)]  veligers 
by  medusae,  and  (3)  rejection,  or  ingestion  and  digestion 
of  oyster  trochophores  and  veligers  by  the  ephyra  and 
scyphistoma  stages  of  C.  quinquecirrha.  We  also  present 
data  on  bivalve  veligers  in  gut  contents  of  medusae  and 
ctenophores,  and  in  situ  densities  of  those  predators  and 
veligers,  to  estimate  the  importance  of  predation  by  ge- 
latinous zooplankton  on  bivalve  larvae  in  the  mesohaline 
region  of  Chesapeake  Bay. 

Materials  and  Methods 

During  June  through  August,  1987,  1988,  and  1989, 
C.  quinquecirrha  medusae  and  M.  leidyi  were  collected 
in  jars  from  the  boat  basin  of  the  Horn  Point  Environ- 
mental Laboratories  (HPEL)  on  the  Choptank  River.  In 
the  laboratory,  we  used  30  nm  filtered  Choptank  River 
water  at  ambient  salinity  (11-1 2%o)  and  temperature  (20- 
27°C).  After  collection,  medusae  and  ctenophores  were 
held  in  20-1  plastic  containers  of  water,  and  fed  on  Anemia 
salina  nauplii  for  at  least  1 2  h  to  clear  their  guts  of  natural 
zooplankton.  Oyster  larvae  from  trochophore  (60  ^m 
long)  to  pediveliger  (270  ^m)  stages,  and  clam  veligers 
(100-260  ^m)  were  obtained  from  the  HPEL  hatchery. 
For  the  following  experiments,  veligers  were  separated  into 
size  fractions  on  screens  of  different  mesh  sizes.  Mussel 
veligers  ( 1 80  ^m)  were  supplied  by  the  University  of  Del- 
aware, College  of  Marine  Studies  in  Lewes,  DE. 

Digestion  and  survival  of  oyster  veligers  after  capture 
by  medusae  and  ctenophores 

Individual  medusae  and  ctenophores  were  exposed  for 
10  min  either  to  high  densities  of  oyster  veligers  alone  (2- 
9  ml"1),  or  to  oyster  veligers  (0.1  ml"1)  with  copepods 


(Acartiu  tonsa)  as  alternative  prey  in  4-1  containers.  The 
predators  then  were  gently  transferred  twice  with  sieves 
(1  mm  mesh)  at  5-min  intervals  to  4-1  containers  with 
filtered  water  to  remove  prey  adhering  to  their  external 
surfaces  and  to  dilute  swimming  zooplankton  possibly 
transferred  with  the  predators.  Each  predator  was  subse- 
quently transferred  at  hourly  intervals  to  new  containers 
of  filtered  water.  After  the  predator  was  removed  from 
each  container,  the  water  was  poured  through  a  60-^m 
screen,  and  live  oyster  veligers,  larval  shells,  live  copepods, 
and  copepod  exoskeletons  were  counted  with  a  dissecting 
microscope,  thus  recording  all  prey  egested  each  hour. 
Egestion  times  were  calculated  from  the  midpoint  of  each 
interval,  so  the  accuracy  is  ±0.5  h.  Living  veligers  that 
were  retrieved  after  egestion  by  the  medusae  were  put  in 
beakers  of  water  with  food  (phytoplankton  Isochrysis  gal- 
bana)  to  determine  their  survival  after  24  h. 

Rejection  and  ingestion  oj  bivalve  veligers  by  medusae 

To  examine  the  feeding  reactions  of  C.  quinquecirrha 
medusae  to  bivalve  veligers  and  copepods,  we  placed  me- 
dusae ( 15-90  mm  in  bell  diameter)  exumbrellar  surface 
down  in  fingerbowls  with  less  than  100  ml  water.  In  this 
position,  medusae  continued  to  take  food,  and  were  easily 
examined  with  a  dissecting  microscope.  Individual  prey 
were  placed  by  pipette  on  the  oral  arms,  where  prey  are 
captured  and  transferred  to  the  gastric  pouches  (Larson, 
1986).  The  length  of  time  it  took  prey  to  reach  a  gastric 
pouch  (ingestion)  or  to  be  rejected  from  the  oral  arm  was 
measured  during  continuous  observation. 

Prey  in  this  experiment  included  live  (closed)  and 
freshly  killed  (gaping)  oyster  veligers,  live  clam  and  mussel 
veligers,  live  and  heat-killed  copepods  (Acartia  tonsa).  and 
tissue  removed  from  2-  to  3-day-old  oyster  spat  (recently 
settled  larvae).  Gaping  veligers  were  used  to  determine 
whether  the  larval  shell  caused  the  rejection  of  veligers  by 
medusae.  To  obtain  gaping  veligers,  we  anaesthetized 
them  by  gradually  adding  seltzer  water  (CO2)  until  the 
shells  opened,  and  then  rapidly  heating  the  water  to  kill 
them.  To  ensure  that  the  medusae  were  feeding  well,  live 
copepods,  which  were  readily  accepted,  were  alternated 
with  other  prey. 

C.  quinquecirrha  ephyrae  2  to  3  mm  in  diameter,  bud- 
ded from  scyphistomae  in  the  laboratory,  were  placed 
singly  in  a  depression  slide  with  0.5  ml  of  water  and  a  few 
live  oyster  trochophores  or  live  oyster  or  clam  veligers; 
the  process  of  rejection  or  ingestion  was  timed  after  con- 
tact occurred.  Scyphistomae  attached  to  plastic  slides  in 
the  laboratory  were  offered  live  oyster  trochophores  or 
veligers  in  25-ml  dishes,  and  rejection  or  ingestion  was 
timed  after  contact. 

Effect  of  scyphistomae  on  veliger  settlement 

To  determine  if  C.  quinquecirrha  scyphistomae  reduced 
oyster  settlement,  field-collected  oyster  shells  containing 


PREDATION  ON  BIVALVE  LARVAE 


105 


scyphistomae  were  cut  into  5  to  8  crrr  pieces  and  cleaned 
of  other  epifauna.  Seven  pieces  of  shell  with  scyphistomae 
(9.3  ±  3.7  individuals  per  shell  for  all  experiments)  and 
seven  without  were  placed  in  3  1  of  1  \%«  water  at  24°  to 
27 °C  in  dishes  of  143  cm:  bottom  area.  Shell  pieces  were 
oriented  so  that  scyphistomae  were  on  the  underside, 
which  is  their  preferred  location  in  nature  (Cargo  and 
Schultz.  1966.  1967).  About  500  oyster  pediveligers  (179- 
250  ^m  long)  were  added  to  the  dishes,  plus  algae  (Iso- 
chrysis  galbana)  as  food  for  the  larvae  and  Anemia  salina 
nauplii  as  alternate  prey  for  the  scyphistomae.  The  dishes 
were  gently  aerated  and  were  covered  with  black  plastic, 
because  oyster  veligers  prefer  low  light  levels  for  settlement 
(Ritchie  and  Menzel,  1969).  The  shell  pieces  were  checked 
at  24  and  48  h  for  newly  settled  larvae.  Six  trials,  each 
with  two  replicates,  were  run  with  different  pieces  of  shell. 
There  were  4  controls,  each  with  14  shell  pieces  without 
scyphistomae. 

Scyphistoma  predation  and  digestion  rales  on  veligers 

Predation  by  scyphistomae  on  oyster  veligers  was  de- 
termined at  the  end  of  each  trial  (24  or  48  h)  by  counting 
the  empty  larval  shells  retrieved  from  the  experimental 
containers.  In  additional  predation  experiments  at  the 
Chesapeake  Biological  Laboratory  (CBL),  containers  were 
filled  with  150  ml  of  estuary  (Patuxent  River)  water.  Each 
container  had  one  plastic  slide  that  was  raised  off  the  bot- 
tom by  fishing  weights  so  that  the  3  to  20  attached  scy- 
phistomae were  on  the  lower  surface.  Fifty  oyster  veligers 
( 179  to  250  jum  long)  and  algal  food  were  added  to  each 
container.  After  24  and  48  h,  larvae  inside  scyphistomae 
and  clear  shells  were  counted.  There  were  159  trials,  and 
26  controls  without  scyphistomae  to  check  for  veliger 
death  due  to  experimental  manipulations.  In  combination 
with  the  preceding  experiment.  171  predation  measure- 
ments were  taken. 

The  length  of  time  required  by  scyphistomae  for  diges- 
tion of  both  closed  (live)  and  gaping  (anaesthetized  and 
killed)  oyster  larvae  was  determined  by  pipetting  the  larvae 
into  the  tentacles  and  mouth  region  of  the  scyphistomae. 
The  times  of  ingestion  were  recorded,  then  containers  were 
checked  at  intervals  for  empty  larval  shells. 

Field  studies  on  medusae  and  ctenophores 

In  1987,  we  sampled  medusae,  ctenophores,  and  bivalve 
veligers  weekly  from  May  to  September  in  two  tributaries 
of  Chesapeake  Bay  [Broad  Creek  (38°  40',  76°15'W)  and 
Tred  Avon  River  (38°40'N,  76°05'W)],  and  on  three  dates 
in  both  May  and  August,  and  on  one  day  in  both  June 
and  July  at  five  stations  across  the  Bay  at  the  same  latitude. 
At  each  station,  we  collected  individual  medusae  and 
ctenophores  by  dip  net  and  immediately  preserved  them 
in  5%  formalin  for  dietary  analysis  with  a  dissecting  mi- 


croscope. All  bivalve  veligers  in  these  samples  were 
counted.  Empty  and  open  larval  shells  were  counted  sep- 
arately from  closed  shells  that  contained  tissue. 

Densities  of  C.  quinquecirrha  and  M.  leidyi  were  mea- 
sured with  a  1  m  diameter,  1.6-mm  mesh  net  with  flow- 
meter  towed  at  1  m  depth  in  the  tributaries  (bottom  depth 
<  4  m),  and  above  the  pycnocline  in  the  Bay  (<1 1  m). 
Medusae  and  ctenophores  were  counted  from  samples 
preserved  in  5%  formalin  (Purcell,  1988).  Densities  of  bi- 
valve larvae  were  determined  from  plankton  samples 
taken  at  the  same  times  as  the  net  tows  at  1  m  depth  in 
the  tributaries  with  a  portable  bilge  pump,  and  at  1-m 
intervals  above  1 1  m  depth  in  the  Bay  with  a  submersible 
pump.  Pump  samples  were  filtered  through  a  64  /urn 
plankton  net  in  the  field,  then  preserved  in  5%  formalin, 
and  veligers  were  counted  in  the  laboratory  from  whole 
samples  or  subsamples  taken  with  a  Hensen  Stempel  pi- 
pette. 

Rates  of  ctenophores  feeding  on  bivalve  veligers  in  sirit 
were  estimated  from  individual  clearance  rates  (Kremer, 
1979)  times  the  numbers  of  ctenophores  per  cubic  meter. 

Statistics 

Our  results  are  presented  as  the  mean  ±  one  standard 
deviation.  Comparisons  on  the  numbers  of  prey  rejected 
or  ingested  were  by  contingency  tables  and  Chi-square 
tests,  and  comparisons  of  the  retention  times  of  different 
prey  species  were  by  one-way  analysis  of  variance.  In  re- 
sults reported  here  as  significantly  different,  the  statistical 
probability  is  less  than  0.001.  unless  stated  otherwise. 

Results 

Digestion  and  survival  of  oyster  veligers  after  capture 
by  medusae  and  ctenophores 

Chrysaora  quinquecirrha  medusae  captured  copepods 
and  oyster  veligers  (80-270  ^m  long).  Ninety-three  percent 
of  the  copepods  were  digested,  compared  with  only  1% 
of  the  veligers  (Table  I).  Medusae  egested  copepod  remains 
in  less  than  5  h.  and  the  few  undigested  copepods  were 


Table  I 

Numbers  of  copepods  and  oyster  ve/igers  digested  after  capture  by 
Chrysaora  quinquecirrha  medusae  and  Mnemiopsis  leidyi 


Predators 

Species 

Captured 

Digested 

tested 

C.  quinquecirrha 

copepods 

12,143 

1  1.276(93%) 

110 

oyster  veligers 

4.800 

48(1%) 

100 

M.  leidvi 

oyster  veligers 

333 

316(96%) 

28 

106 


J.  E.  PURCELL  ET  AL 


Table  II 

Percentages  «!  oyster  vcliKcr.s  o/  different  si:es  sun-iving  for  24  h  after 
cgestion  by  Chrysaora  quinquecirrha  medusae.  Numbers  of  egested 
veligers  are  in  parentheses 


Table  III 

Numbers  of  oyster,  mussel,  and  clam  veligers.  copepods.  and  oyster 
spat  tissue  rejected,  ingested,  and  digested  by  Chrysaora  quinquecirrha 
medusae,  ephyrae.  and  scyphislomae 


Time  inside 

medusa  (h) 

Veliger  size 

<l 

1-2 

2-3 

3-4 

4-5 

5-6 

<100^m 

96.3 

88.9 

75.0 

72.7 



100 

(164) 

(18) 

(12) 

(ID 

(0) 

(2) 

1  00-200  urn 

99.6 

95.2 

97.1 

94.1 

71.4 

91.7 

(1559) 

(272) 

(102) 

(51) 

(14) 

(12) 

>200  fim 

99.4 

100 

93.9 

81.8 

100 

66.7 

(335) 

(65) 

(33) 

(11) 

(6) 

(3) 

dead.  Undigested  veligers  were  egested  in  less  than  7  h, 
with  over  90%  egested  in  less  than  2  h.  Medusae  egested 
shells  of  the  48  veligers  that  were  digested  in  3.4  ±  1.8  h 
at  22  to  27°C.  Digested  veligers  included  31  small  (<100 
j/m)  and  17  medium  (100-200  ^m),  but  no  large  (>200 
veligers.  These  numbers  represent  0.03%,  0.006%>, 


and  0%  of  the  numbers  of  veligers  ingested  in  each  size 
class.  Medusae  digested  veligers  less  than  100  ^m  long 
significantly  more  frequently  than  those  in  both  larger 
size  classes.  More  medium  sized  veligers  were  digested 
than  large  ones  (P  <  0.05).  Ctenophores  digested  signifi- 
cantly more  oyster  veligers  (96%)  than  did  medusae  (1%) 
(Table  I),  and  egested  333  empty  larval  shells  in  2.0  ±  1.0 
hat  19.5  to20.5°C. 

Many  veligers  egested  by  medusae  were  alive.  Overall, 
98.4%  of  2670  veligers  that  we  retrieved  after  egestion  by 
medusae  survived  for  24  h  afterward.  Veligers  smaller  than 
100  /im  long  showed  significantly  lower  survival  than 
larger  veligers  (Table  II).  Veligers  retained  for  more  than 
2  h  showed  significantly  lower  survival  than  those  retained 
for  less  than  2  h  (Table  II).  Differences  between  the  <1  h 
and  the  1-2  h  groups,  and  among  the  groups  >2  h  were 
not  significant  (P  >  0.2  for  all  comparisons). 

Rejection  and  ingestion  of  bivalve  veligers  by  medusae 

Prey  placed  on  an  oral  arm  of  C.  quinquecirrha  medusae 
were  immediately  rejected,  or  they  were  taken  briefly  in- 
side the  oral  arm  by  the  medusae  before  rejection,  or  they 
were  transported  inside  the  oral  arm  and  then  to  a  gastric 
pouch  (ingestion).  Medusae  rejected  significantly  more 
live  oyster  veligers  (99.3%)  from  the  oral  arms  than  live 
copepods  (1.5%)  (Table  III).  The  numbers  of  live  oyster, 
mussel,  and  clam  veligers  rejected  were  not  significantly 
different  (P  =  0.2  to  0.8). 

The  closed  shell  protected  veligers  from  ingestion  and 
digestion  by  medusae.  Open  oyster  veligers  were  rejected 
significantly  less  than  closed,  live  ones,  but  the  difference 
between  closed  and  open  mussel  larvae  was  not  significant 


Prey 

Rejected 

Ingested 

Digested 

Specimens 
tested 

Medusae 

Veligers 

Oyster  —  live 

134 

1 

1 

22 

Oyster  —  dead 

41 

12 

— 

12 

Oyster—  shells 

22 

0 

0 

2 

Mussel  —  live 

91 

4 

— 

8 

Mussel  —  dead 

16 

0 

0 

1 

Clam  —  live 

74 

1 

0 

14 

Oyster  spat  tissue 

6 

27 

>10 

9 

Copepods  —  live 

7 

451 

451 

57 

—  dead 

20 

137 

— 

8 

Ephyrae 

Veligers 

Oyster  —  live 

18 

26 

0 

28 

Clam  —  live 

77 

7 

5 

14 

Trochophores 

3 

117 

2:105 

26 

Scyphistomae 

Veligers 

Oyster  —  live 

9 

32 

12 

19 

Clam  —  live 

9 

8 

7 

12 

=  Not  quantified  because  we  were  unable  to  track  the  prey. 


(Table  III).  Open  oyster  veligers  also  were  retained  sig- 
nificantly longer  in  the  oral  arms  than  were  closed  veligers 
(Table  IV).  Empty  larval  shells  were  never  ingested  (Table 
III).  Oyster  spat  tissue  was  ingested  significantly  more  fre- 
quently than  either  open  or  closed  oyster  veligers  (Table 
III).  Dead  copepods  were  rejected  significantly  more  often 


Table  IV 

Percentages  of  bivalve  veligers  that  were  retained  for  five  time  intervals 
in  l/ie  oral  arms  of  Chrysaora  quinquecirrha  medusae.  The  numbers 
of  veligers  tested  are  in  the  "Rejected"  column  in  Table  III 


Time  inside  oral  arm  (min) 

Maximum  time 

Prey 

<1         1-2 

2-4 

4-10 

>10 

(min) 

Oysters 

live 

16          31 

19 

19 

16 

45 

dead 

12          24 

20 

5 

39 

156 

Mussels 

live 

9         31 

12 

24 

24 

70 

Clams 

live 

2          10 

8 

15 

65 

91 

PREDATION  ON  BIVALVE  LARVAE 


107 


than  live  ones  (Table  III),  but  most  dead  ones  were  still 
accepted  as  food. 

Although  nearly  all  veligers  were  eventually  rejected 
from  the  oral  arms  of  C.  quinquecirrha  medusae,  differ- 
ences in  retention  time  existed  among  the  three  bivalve 
species  tested  (Table  IV).  Most  live  veligers  were  rejected 
in  less  than  10  min.  Live  mussel  veligers  were  retained 
somewhat  longer  than  live  oysters,  but  the  difference  was 
not  significant  (P  =  0.2).  Clams  were  retained  significantly 
longer  before  rejection  than  were  oysters  and  mussels. 

Comparisons  among  life  history  stages  of  C.  quinque- 
cirrha showed  that  ephyrae  and  scyphistomae  ingested 
proportionately  more  oyster  and  clam  veligers  than  did 
the  medusae  (Table  III).  Ingestion  of  oyster  veligers  dif- 
fered significantly  between  medusae  and  ephyrae,  and  be- 
tween medusae  and  scyphistomae;  however,  differences 
between  ephyrae  and  scyphistomae  were  not  significant 
(P  =  0. 1 ).  Ingestion  of  clam  veligers  differed  significantly 
between  scyphistomae  and  medusae,  and  between  scy- 
phistomae and  ephyrae;  however  the  difference  between 
medusae  and  ephyrae  was  not  significant  (P  =  0. 1 ). 

Of  the  ingested  veligers,  scyphistomae  digested  signif- 
icantly more  oysters  than  did  ephyrae  (Table  III),  but  not 
clams  (P  =  0.9).  Thus,  ephyrae  behaved  more  like  me- 
dusae than  scyphistomae  in  that  they  digested  few  oyster 
veligers.  Ephyrae  digested  five  clam  veligers  in  1 .8  to  20.6 
hfmean  10.6  ±  8.3  h). 

Comparisons  between  types  of  veligers  showed  that 
ephyrae  ingested  significantly  more  oyster  than  clam  ve- 
ligers (Table  III),  but  digested  significantly  more  clams 
than  oysters.  In  contrast,  scyphistomae  ingested  signifi- 
cantly more  clam  than  oyster  veligers  (P  <  0.05),  and 
digested  significantly  more  clams  than  oysters  (P  <  0.05). 
These  results  suggest  that  clam  and  oyster  veligers  are 
captured  with  different  success  by  ephyrae  and  scyphis- 
tomae, and  that  oyster  veligers  show  greater  resistance  to 
digestion  than  do  clam  veligers  once  captured. 

Because  individual  oyster  trochophore  larvae  were  dif- 
ficult to  observe  due  to  their  small  size  (<60  ^m),  we  were 
successful  at  offering  them  only  to  ephyrae,  which  ingested 
and  digested  significantly  more  trochophores  than  veligers 
(Table  III). 

Effect  of  scyphistomae  on  veliger  settlement 

No  settlement  of  oyster  veligers  occurred  in  three  of 
six  experiments.  Veligers  in  three  experiments  and  one 
control  settled  preferentially  on  the  lower  surfaces  of  the 
shell  pieces,  even  those  with  C.  quinquecirrha  scyphisto- 
mae. Numbers  of  spat  on  the  upper/lower  shell  surfaces 
were:  shells  with  scyphistomae  19/69;  without  scyphis- 
tomae 22/49;  control  22/77.  No  significant  differences  in 
spat  settlement  were  seen  among  shell  pieces  with  or 
without  scyphistomae.  which  were  on  the  lower  surfaces 


(P  >  0.2  for  all  comparisons).  Total  settlement  was  greater 
in  the  control  container  (average  of  seven  veligers  settled 
per  shell),  where  there  were  no  scyphistomae,  as  compared 
with  the  experimental  containers  (average  settlement  of 
two  per  shell),  probably  because  predation  by  scyphisto- 
mae reduced  the  numbers  of  veligers. 

Scyphistoma  predation  and  digestion  rates  on  veligers 

A  total  of  4409  oyster  veligers  were  consumed  by  Chrv- 
saora  quinquecirrha  scyphistomae  in  171  predation  ex- 
periments, as  evidenced  by  the  presence  of  empty  shells. 
In  contrast,  only  9  empty  shells  were  retrieved  from  27 
controls  without  scyphistomae.  No  significant  differences 
existed  between  the  ingestion  rates  measured  at  24  and 
48  h,  therefore  the  results  were  pooled.  The  initial  densities 
of  larvae  in  the  experimental  and  control  containers  av- 
eraged 0.31  ±  0.06  veligers  ml"1.  Over  the  range  of  prey 
density  (0.1-0.7  veligers  ml1),  the  number  of  larvae  con- 
sumed per  scyphistoma  per  day  (range  0-13)  was  posi- 
tively correlated  with  larval  density  (r  =  0.26,  P  <  0.01). 
On  average,  each  scyphistoma  consumed  0.9  ±  0.6  ve- 
ligers/day.  As  many  as  15  larvae  were  observed  within  a 
single  scyphistoma.  These  results  indicate  that  scyphis- 
tomae are  more  effective  predators  on  oyster  veligers  than 
are  medusae.  However,  we  observed  that  after  a  few  hours, 
scyphistomae  sometimes  expelled  ingested  larvae,  which 
began  swimming  again.  These  larvae  then  were  available 
for  recapture. 

Closed  bivalve  veligers  were  very  resistant  to  digestion 
by  scyphistomae.  Closed  D-stage  clam  veligers  were  di- 
gested in  37.5  to  41  h  (mean  39.2  ±  1.2  h,  n  =  34),  and 
clam  pedi veligers  were  digested  in  4  to  47  h  (mean  30.6 
±  15.6  h,  n  =  6).  Scyphistomae  that  had  ingested  one  or 
two  closed  oyster  pediveligers  egested  empty  shells  in  24 
to  67  h  (mean  34.6  ±  12.9  h,  n  =  13).  Three  pediveligers 
removed  from  scyphistomae  after  18.5  h  appeared  to  be 
healthy.  In  contrast,  open  oyster  pediveligers  were  digested 
in  only  1.3  to  5.1  h  (mean  3.7  ±  0.8  h,  n  =  32). 

Field  studies  on  medusae  and  ctenophores 

Field-collected  M.  leidyi  and  C.  quinquecirrha  medusae 
both  contained  bivalve  veligers.  In  67  medusae,  the  shells 
of  all  77  veligers  were  closed  and  full  of  tissue,  indicating 
that  they  had  not  been  digested.  By  contrast,  19  of  76 
(25%)  of  the  shells  in  9  ctenophores  were  open  and  empty, 
indicating  complete  digestion.  The  proportions  of  open 
and  closed  shells  in  medusae  and  ctenophores  were  sig- 
nificantly different.  Ctenophores  contained  more  veligers 
(an  average  of  six  each)  than  did  medusae  (about  one 
each).  This  may  be  because  the  ctenophores  were  collected 
in  Chesapeake  Bay,  where  veliger  densities  were  much 
greater  than  in  the  tributaries,  which  was  where  the  me- 
dusae were  collected  for  diet  studies  (Table  V). 


108 


J.  E.  PURCELL  ET  AL. 


Table  V 

Densities  (numbers  m'3)  ol  Chrysaora  quinquecirrha  medusae.  Mncmiopsis  leidyi.  and  bivalve  veligers  in  Chesapeake  Bay  and  the  Broad  Creek 
and  Trcd  Avon  River  tributaries  from  May  in  August.  I9S7,  and  ihe  percentages  of  veligers  consumed  per  day  by  Mnemiopsis 


Chesapeake  Bay 


Tributaries 


Veligers* 
consumed 

Veligers* 
consumed 

per  day 

per  day 

Month 

Medusae 

Ctenophores 

Veligers 

(%) 

Medusae 

Ctenophores 

Veligers 

(%) 

May 

0 

0.3  ±  0.5 

13,826  ±  11.491 

0.2  ±  0.2 

0-0.3 

0.2-33.1 

_ 

June 

0 

2.7  ±  1.6 

14.210  ±    8,145 

1.0  ±0.6 

5.4  ±  5.8 

0 

1786  ±  1550 

0 

July 

O.I  ±0.1 

0.1  ±0.1 

60,032  ±  97,380 

0.2  ±  0.2 

9.6  ±  4.2 

0 

419  ±    279 

0 

August 

0.6  ±  0.7 

0.7  ±0.8 

1  2,284  ±  15,234 

1.7  ±  1.9 

7.2  ±  3.7 

0 

1421  ±  1060 

0 

*  Percentage  daily  consumption  estimated  from  ctenophore  filtering  rates  (Kremer.  1979). 
—  =  No  data. 


To  estimate  the  importance  of  predation  on  veligers  by 
medusae  and  Ctenophores  in  nature,  we  measured  ///  situ 
densities  of  M.  leidyi,  C.  quinquecirrha,  and  bivalve  ve- 
ligers in  May  through  August,  1987  (Table  V).  Cteno- 
phores occurred  in  the  Bay  throughout  this  period,  but 
they  were  excluded  from  the  tributaries  by  high  densities 
of  medusae  that  fed  on  them  from  June  through  August. 
Medusae  were  much  less  abundant  in  the  Bay  than  in  the 
tributaries.  Sampled  densities  of  bivalve  veligers  were 
much  greater  in  the  Bay  than  in  the  tributaries,  possibly 
due  to  different  efficiencies  of  the  pumps  used  to  collect 
them.  If  we  assume  that  only  Ctenophores  ate  the  bivalve 
veligers,  then  0.2  to  1.7%  of  the  veligers  were  consumed 
daily  in  the  main  Bay,  and  none  were  eaten  in  the  trib- 
utaries during  that  period  (Table  V). 

Discussion 

A  surprising  result  of  this  study  is  that  Chrysaora  quin- 
quecirrha medusae  do  not  ingest  or  digest  bivalve  veliger 
larvae.  Three  lines  of  evidence  lead  to  this  conclusion.  ( 1 ) 
Medusae  that  caught  swimming  veligers  egested  them 
alive.  (2)  Veligers  placed  on  oral  arms  were  subsequently 
rejected.  (3)  Veligers  in  the  gut  contents  of  field-collected 
medusae  were  closed  and  full  of  tissue.  The  ephyra  stage 
ingested  oyster  veligers  but  did  not  digest  them.  By  con- 
trast, scyphistomae  egested  some  living  veligers,  but  many 
were  retained  and  eventually  digested. 

The  larval  shell  may  protect  bivalve  veligers  from 
ingestion  by  C.  quinquecirrha  medusae.  The  rapid  rejec- 
tion of  veligers  from  the  oral  arms  suggests  that  medusae 
either  do  not  recognize  veligers  as  food  items  because  of 
the  shell,  or  that  veligers  provide  a  "distasteful"  stimulus. 
Larvae  of  an  echinoderm  (Acanthaster  planci)  and  an  as- 
cidian  (Ecteinascidia  turbinata)  contain  chemicals  that 
make  them  unpalatable  to  planktivorous  fishes  (Lucas  et 
a/..  1979;  Young  and  Bingham,  1987). 


The  sensing  and  recognition  of  food  must  take  place 
in  the  oral  arms  of  the  medusae,  as  indicated  by  the  dif- 
ferences in  ingestion  of  copepods  and  veliger  larvae.  This 
recognition  may  involve  a  mechanical  stimulus  from  ac- 
tive prey,  as  suggested  by  the  facts  that  more  living,  active 
copepods  were  ingested  than  dead  ones,  and  that  immobile 
veligers  nearly  always  were  rejected.  Recognition  also  may 
be  due  to  chemical  stimuli,  because  more  open  oyster 
veligers.  which  presumably  leaked  body  fluids,  were  in- 
gested than  closed  ones.  The  various  bivalve  species  also 
may  present  different  stimuli,  as  suggested  by  the  different 
retention  times  of  oyster,  mussel,  and  clam  veligers  in  the 
medusae. 

The  larval  shell  probably  protects  veligers  from  diges- 
tion as  long  as  they  remain  closed  within  the  predators. 
Veligers  were  retained  for  up  to  7  h  in  medusae,  and  then 
egested  alive.  Veligers  were  removed  alive  from  scyphis- 
tomae after  1 8  h,  but  closed  oyster  pediveligers  eventually 
were  digested  in  over  24  h.  By  contrast,  newly  killed  ve- 
ligers with  open  shells  were  digested  by  scyphistomae  in 
3  to  5  h.  Therefore,  open  veligers  apparently  are  more 
susceptible  to  digestion  than  closed  ones.  Digestion  of 
some  veligers  may  be  due  to  their  injury  by  the  scyphis- 
tomae's  nematocysts  at  capture,  causing  the  shells  to  open. 
Presumably,  this  also  could  explain  why  a  few  veligers 
were  digested  by  the  medusae. 

Suspension-feeding  benthic  invertebrates  can  be  im- 
portant predators  of  pelagic  larvae  (Thorson,  1946).  Bi- 
valve larvae  have  been  found  in  the  stomach  contents  of 
their  own  and  other  bivalve  species  (summarized  in  Mil- 
eikovsky,  1974;  Young  and  Chia,  1987).  However,  oyster 
larvae  taken  into  the  mantle  cavities  of  six  mollusk  species 
were  rejected  in  the  pseudofeces,  from  which  they  may 
be  able  to  escape  (MacKenzie,  1981).  A  few  veligers  were 
ingested  and  eliminated  in  the  feces  of  these  mollusks, 
from  which  they  could  not  escape  (MacKenzie,  1981). 


PREDATION  ON   BIVALVE  LARVA! 


109 


Oyster  veligers  also  were  rejected  unharmed  by  a  barnacle 
(Balanus  eburneus)  and  a  polychaete  (Polydora  ligni)  (in 
MacKenzie,  1981),  but  the  common  barnacle  (Balanus 
improvisus)  ate  oyster  veligers  in  Chesapeake  Bay  (Stein- 
berg and  Kennedy,  1979). 

From  earlier  studies,  Mileikovsky  ( 1 974)  concluded  that 
bivalve  veligers  often  could  pass  alive  through  the  guts  of 
primarily  herbivorous  feeders.  However,  no  larvae  were 
known  to  pass  alive  through  primarily  carnivorous  feeders, 
although  protectively  coated  gametes  of  a  polychaete 
(Melinna  palnmta)  passed  through  fish  (Acipenser  stella- 
tus)  feeding  on  the  adult  worms  (in  Mileikovsky,  1974). 
Numerous  examples  exist  of  benthic  cnidarians  feeding 
on  bivalve  veligers  (Young  and  Chia,  1987).  Also,  oyster 
veligers  were  eaten  by  the  common  sea  anemone  Dia- 
dumene  leucolena  in  Chesapeake  Bay  (Steinberg  and 
Kennedy,  1979).  To  our  knowledge,  our  study  presents 
the  first  evidence  of  bivalve  veligers  passing  alive  through 
a  carnivorous  predator,  the  medusa  stage  of  Chrysaora 
quinquecirrha. 

The  diets  of  several  species  of  pelagic  cnidarians  are 
reported  to  include  bivalve  veligers,  but  the  numbers  of 
veligers  in  siphonophores  (Purcell,  1981)  and  hydrome- 
dusae  (reviewed  in  Purcell  and  Mills,  1988)  usually  were 
less  than  1%  of  the  prey  items.  Similarly,  the  scyphome- 
dusae  Aurclia  aurita  and  Stomolophux  meleagris  in  the 
Gulf  of  Mexico,  and  Mastigias  sp.  in  Jellyfish  Lake.  Palau, 
contained  small  numbers  of  bivalve  veligers  (Purcell,  un- 
pub.  data).  However,  bivalve  veligers  were  25  to  67%  of 
the  prey  in  the  hydromedusan  Proboscidactylaflavicirrata 
(Purcell  and  Mills,  1988),  and  40  to  80%  of  the  prey  in 
the  scyphomedusan  Cyanea  sp.  (Brewer,  1989).  None  of 
the  above  studies  distinguished  between  digested  or  un- 
digested veligers. 

The  importance  of  predation  on  oyster  larvae  by  scy- 
phistomae  in  nature  is  difficult  to  predict  because  there 
are  few  density  estimates  for  scyphistomae  or  for  oyster 
veligers  near  the  estuary  bottom.  Only  2.8  ±  3.1%  of  oyster 
shells  had  scyphistomae  in  the  York  River,  Virginia 
(Cones  and  Haven,  1969).  One  third  of  those  shells  had 
an  average  of  more  than  10  scyphistomae  per  shell  (max- 
imum 2 1 ),  and  densities  were  <1  to  53  scyphistomae  m"2 
of  bottom.  However,  53.4  ±  25.3%  of  oyster  shells  con- 
tained scyphistomae  in  eleven  tributaries  of  the  Chesa- 
peake Bay  in  Maryland,  and  70%  of  those  shells  had  more 
than  10  individuals  (maximum  200;  Cargo,  unpub.  data). 
Predation  by  scyphistomae  on  oyster  veligers  in  those 
tributaries  probably  would  be  higher  than  in  the  York 
River. 

The  predation  rate  of  one  oyster  veliger  scyphistoma"1 
day"1  from  our  laboratory  experiments  should  be  applied 
to  field  conditions  with  caution,  because  the  experimental 
larval  densities  (100-700  1~',  mean  300  I"1)  were  generally 
high  in  comparison  with  densities  of  pediveligers  in  bot- 


tom waters.  Oyster  veliger  densities  were  generally  less 
than  14  T '  near  the  bottom  in  Broad  Creek  and  the  Tred 
Avon  River,  but  one  sample  had  134  1'  (Seliger  el  a!.. 
1982).  Densities  of  oyster  veligers  >  200  nm  long  were 
23  to  2 1 5  r '  near  the  bottom  in  the  James  River,  Virginia 
(Andrews,  1983).  Mortality  in  our  laboratory  experiments 
could  be  higher  than  in  the  field  because  veligers  that  were 
expelled  undigested  by  scyphistomae  in  our  experiments 
could  have  been  repeatedly  ingested,  eventually  resulting 
in  death,  while  veligers  in  nature  might  have  escaped. 

Molluscan  trochophore  larvae  lack  a  shell,  and  are 
probably  vulnerable  to  predation  by  all  life  history  stages 
of  C.  quinquecirrha.  We  could  only  follow  the  fate  of 
trochophores  offered  to  ephyrae,  which  did  ingest  and  di- 
gest them.  In  nature,  trochophores  may  be  distributed 
throughout  the  water  column,  and  may  seldom  encounter 
benthic  scyphistomae.  Although  medusae  do  consume 
some  copepod  nauplii  and  rotifers  of  the  same  size  as 
trochophores  (about  60  ^m),  such  small  animals  were  only 
a  few  percent  of  the  prey  items  (Purcell,  unpub.  data). 
Therefore,  medusae  probably  do  not  capture  many 
trochophores  in  nature.  Depending  on  temperature,  the 
trochophore  stage  lasts  only  24  to  30  h,  so  this  period  of 
vulnerability  to  predators  is  short,  compared  with  the  6 
to  1 8  day  veliger  stage  of  various  bivalve  species  (Loosanoft" 
and  Davis,  1963).  Ctenophores  readily  ingested  and  di- 
gested veligers,  and  they  probably  also  eat  trochophores, 
because  they  consume  many  copepod  nauplii  (Purcell, 
unpub.  data)  and  ciliates  (Stoecker  et  a/..  1987)  of  the 
same  size. 

Quaglietta  ( 1987)  studied  potential  predation  by  Alne- 
miopsis  leidyi  on  larvae  of  the  hard  clam  Mercenaria 
mercenaria  in  Great  South  Bay,  New  York.  Clam  veligers 
and  Ctenophores  co-occurred  in  July  through  December, 
and  were  most  abundant  in  August  through  September. 
Ctenophore  feeding  reached  a  maximum  in  September, 
with  an  average  of  1 1  and  36%  of  the  water  cleared  of 
prey  per  day  in  1985  and  1986,  respectively.  Both  the 
biomass  of  Ctenophores  and  their  estimated  predation 
on  veligers  were  greater  during  Quaglietta's  (1987)  study 
in  Great  South  Bay  than  during  our  study  in  Chesa- 
peake Bay. 

Predation  on  bivalve  veligers  by  M.  leidyi  during  our 
study  was  apparently  limited  to  Chesapeake  Bay,  because 
the  Ctenophores  were  not  found  in  Broad  Creek  and  Tred 
Avon  River  after  the  appearance  of  C  quinquecirrha  me- 
dusae in  June.  Predation  by  medusae  on  M.  leidyi  also 
may  have  reduced  ctenophore  densities  in  the  main  Bay. 
We  conclude  that  not  only  do  C.  quinquecirrha  medusae 
not  consume  bivalve  veligers.  but  the  medusae  may  reduce 
other  predation  on  them  by  feeding  on  Ctenophores. 

In  the  mesohaline  region  of  Chesapeake  Bay,  C.  quin- 
quecirrha medusae  are  present  during  June  through  Sep- 
tember or  October  (Cargo  and  Schultz,  1966).  Therefore, 


10 


J.  E.  PURCELL  /•/     II 


medusae  could  reduce  ctenophore  predation  on  veligers 
ofCrassostrea  virginica,  as  well  as  other  bivalves  such  as 
Ischadium  recumtm  Raftnesque,  Macoma  nuic/ielli  Dall. 
Mulinia  lalcralis.  Mytilopsis  congeria  (Conrad),  and  Ta- 
gflux  plebeius  (Lightfoot)  which  spawn  throughout  the 
summer  (Shaw,  1965;  Kennedy,  pers.  obs.).  However,  bi- 
valve species  that  spawn  only  in  the  spring  and  autumn 
in  Chesapeake  Bay,  e.g..  Macoma  balthica  (L.)  and  Mya 
arenaria  (L.)  (Shaw,  1965),  would  be  most  vulnerable  to 
predation  by  M.  Icidyi. 

Acknowledgments 

We  thank  T.  Dean,  C.  Densmore,  C.  Kalafus,  L.  Hill, 
and  V.  Steele-Perkins  for  their  excellent  assistance  in  the 
laboratory,  and  C.  A.  Miller  and  J.  R.  White  for  their 
comments  on  the  manuscript.  We  are  also  grateful  to  Drs. 
R.  I.  E.  Newell,  G.  S.  Alspach,  and  T.  C.  Malone  for 
allowing  us  to  sample  during  their  cruises,  to  Dr.  J.  H. 
Waite  of  the  College  of  Marine  Studies  of  the  University 
of  Delaware  for  providing  mussel  veligers,  and  to  Dr. 
M.  R.  Roman  and  J.  R.  White,  who  provided  data  on 
bivalve  veliger  densities  from  Chesapeake  Bay.  This  re- 
search was  partially  funded  by  the  Maryland  Department 
of  Natural  Resources,  and  the  NSF  Research  Experiences 
for  Undergraduates  Grant  OCE-8900707  to  the  University 
of  Maryland  Sea  Grant  College.  UMCEES  Contribution 
No.  2175. 

Literature  Cited 

Andrews,  J.  D.  1983.  Transport  of  bivalve  larvae  in  James  River,  Vir- 
ginia. /  Shellfish  Res.  3:  29-40. 

Bishop,  J.  \V.  1967.  Feeding  rates  of  the  ctenophore,  Mnemiopsis  Icidyi. 
Chesapeake  Sci.  8:  259-261. 

Brewer,  R.  H.  1989.  The  annual  pattern  of  feeding,  growth,  and  sexual 
reproduction  in  Cyanea  (CnidariaiScyphozoa)  in  the  Niantic  River 
Estuary,  Connecticut.  Biol.  Bull.  176:  272-281. 

Burrell,  V.  G.,  Jr.  1968.  The  ecological  significance  of  the  ctenophore 
(Mnemiopsis  leiityi  A.  Agassiz)  in  a  fish  nursery  grounds.  M.  S.  Thesis. 
College  of  William  and  Mary.  Williamsburg.  VA.  61  pp. 

Burrell,  V.  B.,  Jr.,  and  W.  A.  Van  Engel.  1976.  Predation  by  and  dis- 
tribution of  a  ctenophore,  Mnemiopsis  Icidyi  A.  Agassiz,  in  the  York 
Estuary.  Estuarine  Coastal  Mar.  Sci.  4:  235-242. 

Cargo,  D.  G.,  and  I..  P.  Schultz.  1966.  Notes  on  the  biology  of  the  sea 
nettle.  Chrysaora  quinquecirrha,  in  Chesapeake  Bay.  Chesapeake  Sci. 
7:95-100. 

Cargo,  D.  G.,  and  I..  P.  Schultz.  1967.  Further  observations  on  the 
biology  of  the  sea  nettle  and  jellylishes  in  Chesapeake  Bay.  Chesapeake 
Sci.  8:  209-220. 

Clifford,  H.  C.,  and  D.  G.  Cargo.  1978.  Feeding  rates  of  the  sea  nettle 
Chrysaora  quinquecirrha,  under  laboratory  conditions.  Estuaries  I: 
58-61. 

Cones,  H.  N.,  Jr.,  and  D.  S.  Haven.  1969.  Distribution  of  Chrysaora 
quinquecirrha  in  the  York  River.  Chesapeake  Sci.  10:  75-84. 

Cronin,  L.  E.,  J.  C.  Daiber,  and  E.  M.  Ilulbert.  1962.  Quantitative 
seasonal  aspects  of  zooplankton  in  the  Delaware  River  estuary.  Ches- 
apeake Sci.  3:  63-93. 


Deason,  E.  E.,  andT.  J.  Smayda.  1982.     Ctenophore-zooplankton-phy- 

toplankton  interactions  in  Narragansett  Bay,  Rhode  Island,  USA, 

during  1972-1977.  Plankton  Res.  2:  203-218. 
Fancett.  M.  S.  1988.     Diet  and  prey  selectivity  of  scyphomedusae  from 

Port  Phillip  Bay,  Australia.  Mar.  Biol.  98:  503-509. 
Feigcnbaum,  D.,  and  M.  Kell).  1984.     Changes  in  the  lower  Chesapeake 

Bay  food  chain  in  presence  of  the  sea  nettle,  Chrysaora  quinquecirrha 

(Scyphomedusa).  Mar.  Ecol.  Prog.  Ser.  19:  39-47. 
Herman,   S.   S.,  J.   A.   Mihursky,   and   A.   M.   McErlean.    1968. 

Zooplankton  and  environmental  characteristics  of  the  Patuxent  River 

estuary  1963-1965.  Chesapeake  Sci.  9:  67-82. 
Kremcr,  P.  1979.     Predation  by  the  ctenophore  Mnemiopsis  Icidyi  in 

Narragansett  Bay.  R.  I.  Estuaries  2:  97-105. 
Kenned),  V.  S.,  and  L.  L.  Breisch.  1981.     Mary/anil's  Oysters:  Research 

and  Management.  Univ.  of  MD  Sea  Grant  Program.  College  Park. 

MD  Publ.  No.  UM-SG-TS-81-04. 
Larson,  R.  J.  1978.     Aspects  of  feeding  and  functional  morphology  of 

scyphomedusae.  M.  S.  Thesis,  University  of  Puerto  Rico.  132  pp. 
Larson,  R.  J.  1986.     The  feeding  and  growth  of  the  sea  nettle.  Chrysaora 

quinquecirrha  (Desor),  in  the  laboratory.  Estuaries  9:  376-379. 
Larson,  R.  J.  1987.     A  note  on  the  feeding,  growth,  and  reproduction 

of  the  epipelagic  scyphomedusa  Pelagia  nocliluca  (Foskal).  Biol. 

Oceanogr  4:  447-454. 
Loosanoff.  V.  L.  1974.     Factors  responsible  for  the  mass  mortalities  of 

molluscan  larvae  in  nature:  a  review.  Proc.  Ann.  Mlg.  World  Mari- 

cultitre  Soc.  5:  297-309. 
LoosanofT,  V.  L.,  and  II.  C.  Davis.  1963.     Rearing  of  bivalve  mollusks. 

,\d\    Mar.  Biol.  \.  1-135. 
Lucas,  J.  S.,  R.  J.  Hart,  M.  E.  Howden,  and  R.  Salathe.  1979.     Saponins 

in  eggs  and  larvae  of  Acanthasler  planci  (Asteroidea)  as  chemical 

defenses  against  planktivorous  fish.  /  Exp.  Afar.  Biol.  Ecol.  40:  155- 

165. 

Mackenzie,  C.  L.,  Jr.  1981.     Biotic  potential  and  environmental  resis- 
tance in  the  American  oyster  (Crassostrea  virginica)  in  Long  Island 

Sound.  Aatiacttliitre  22:  229-268. 
Mileikovsky,  S.  A.  1974.     On  predation  of  pelagic  larvae  and  early 

juveniles  of  marine  bottom  invertebrates  by  adult  benthic  inverte- 
brates and  their  passing  alive  through  their  predators.  Mar.  Biol  26: 

303-311. 
Miller,  R.  J.  1974.     Distribution  and  biomass  of  an  estuarine  ctenophore 

population,  Mnemiopsis  leidyi  (A.  Agassiz).  Chesapeake  Sci.  15:  1-8. 
Nelson,  T.  C.  1925.     On  the  occurrence  and  food  habits  of  ctenophores 

in  New  Jersey  inland  coastal  waters.  Biol.  Bull  48:  92-1 1  1. 
Nelson,  T.  C.  1953.     Some  observations  on  the  migrations  and  setting 

of  oyster  larvae.  Proi:  .\'all.  Shellfish.  Assoc.  (1952):  99-104. 
Olson,  M.  M.  1987.     Zooplankton.  Pp.  38-8 1  in  Ecological  Studies  in 

the  Middle  Reach  ol  Chesapeake  Bay.  K.  L.  Heck.  Jr.,  ed.  Springer 

Verlag.  Berlin. 
Purcell,  J.  E.  1981.     Dietary  composition  and  diel  feeding  patterns  of 

epipelagic  siphonophores.  Mar.  Biol.  65:  83-90. 
Purcell,  J.  E.  1988.     Quantification  of  Mnemiopsis  leidyi  (Ctenophora, 

Lobata)  from  formalin  preserved  plankton  samples.  Mar.  Ecol.  Prog. 

Ser.  45:  197-200. 
Purcell,  J.  E.,  and  C.  E.  Mills.  1988.     The  correlation  between  nema- 

tocyst  types  and  diets  in  pelagic  Hydrozoa.  Pp.  463-485  in  The  Bi- 
ology of  Nematocyst.s.  D.  A.  Hessinger  and  H.  M.  Lenhoff.  eds.  Ac- 
ademic Press,  Inc..  San  Diego. 
Quaglietla,  C.  E.  1987.     Predation  by  Mnemiopsis  Icidyi  on  hard  clam 

larvae  and  other  natural  zooplankton  in  Great  South  Bay.  N.Y. 

M.  S.  Thesis,  State  University  of  New  York,  Stony  Brook.  66  pp. 
Ritchie,  T.  P.,  and  R.  W.  Menzel.  1969.     Influence  of  light  on  larval 

settlement  of  American  oyster.  Proc.  Nail.  Shellfish.  Assoc.  59:  1 16- 

120. 


PREDATION  ON  BIVALVE  LARVAE 


1 


Seliger,  H.  H.,  J.  A.  Boggs,  R.  B.  Rivkin,  W.  H.  Biggley,  and  K.  R.  II. 

Aspden.  1982.     The  transpon  of  oyster  larvae  in  an  estuary.  Mar. 

Biol  71:  57-72, 
Shaw,  \V.  N.  1965.     Seasonal  setting  patterns  of  five  species  of  bivalves 

in  the  Tred  Avon  River.  Maryland.  Chesapeake  Set.  6:  33-37. 
Steinberg,  P.  D.,  and  V.  S.  Kennedy.  1979.     Predation  upon  Crassostrea 

virginicu  (Gmelin)  larvae  by  two  invertebrate  species  common  to 

Chesapeake  Bay  oyster  bars.  I  'eliger  22:  78-84. 
Sloecker,  D.  K.,  P.  G.  Verity,  A.  E.  Michaels,  and   I..  H.  Davis. 

1987.     Feeding  by  larval  and  post-larval  ctenophores  on  microzoo- 

plankton.  /  Plankton  Res  9:  667-683. 
Thorson,  G.  1946.     Reproduction  and  larval  development  of  Danish 

marine  bottom  invertebrates  with  special  reference  to  planktomc  lar- 


vae in  the  Sound  (Oresund).  Meddr  Damn   l-'isk.  -og  Havunders. 
(Ser.  Plankton)  4:  1-523. 

I  ruitt,  R.  V.,  and  P.  V.  Mook.  1925.  Oyster  problem  inquiry  of  Ches- 
apeake Bay.  Third  Ann  Rc;>  Consent.  Dept  oj  MD  Pp.  25-55. 

Young,  C.  M.,  and  B.  1,.  Bingham.  1987.  Chemical  defense  and  apo- 
sematic  coloration  in  larvae  of  the  ascidian  Ecteinascidin  turbinata 
Mar.  Bio/  96:  539-544. 

Young,  C.  M.,  and  K.-S.  C'hia.  1987.  Abundance  and  distribution  of 
pelagic  larvae  as  influenced  by  predation,  behavior,  and  hydrographic 
factors.  Pp.  385-463  in  Reproduction  of  Marine  Invertebrates, 
\'ol.  IX  General  aspects:  seeking  unity  in  diversity.  A.  C. 
Giese,  J.  S.  Pearse,  and  V.  B.  Pearse.  eds.  Boxwood  Press,  Pacific 
Grove,  CA. 


Reference:  Biol.  Bull  180:  112-118.  (February,  1991) 


Settlement,  Refuges,  and  Adult  Body  Form  in  Colonial 
Marine  Invertebrates:  A  Field  Experiment 


LINDA  J.  WALTERS'   AND  DAVID  S.  WETHEY12 

1  Department  of  Biological  Sciences  and  -Marine  Science  Program,  University  of  South  Carolina. 

Columbia,  South  Carolina  29208 


Abstract.  We  examine  the  relationship  between  adult 
body  form  (sheet  vs.  arborescent)  and  larval  settlement 
in  colonial  animals.  Because  thin  sheet  forms  are  more 
susceptible  to  overgrowth  than  arborescent  forms,  we 
predict  that  larvae  of  sheet  forms  should  preferentially 
settle  in  refuges  from  competitors.  On  both  natural  and 
artificial  substrata,  the  larvae  of  the  sheet  form  (Mem- 
branipora  membranacea)  settled  more  often  on  high  spots, 
which  could  serve  as  refuges  from  competition.  The  ar- 
borescent forms  (Bitgula  ncrilina  and  Distaplia  occiden- 
talis)  settled  around  the  bases  of  bumps  more  frequently 
than  would  be  expected  by  chance.  For  many  arborescent 
forms,  their  most  vulnerable  periods  are  the  days  im- 
mediately following  settlement,  when  individuals  can  be 
consumed  easily  by  predators  or  dislodged  by  physical 
disturbances.  Settlement  in  a  crevice  (base  of  a  bump) 
would  provide  protection  from  the  bulky  mouthparts  of 
predators.  Moreover,  dislodgment  would  be  less  likely 
than  if  settlement  had  occurred  on  flat  locations,  such  as 
the  tops  of  bumps  or  the  areas  between  bumps. 

Introduction 

Striking  patterns  of  spatial  distribution  are  characteristic 
of  many  marine  invertebrates  sessile  on  algae,  rocks,  and 
other  hard  surfaces.  Individuals  are  often  found  in  aggre- 
gations relative  to  each  other  (e.g.,  Knight-Jones,  1951; 
Crisp,  1961;  Wethey,  1984),  relative  to  topographic  fea- 
tures of  the  substrata  (e.g.,  Crisp  and  Barnes,  1954;  Ry- 
land,  1959;  Crisp,  1961;  Wisely,  1960;  Hayward  and 
Harvey,  1974;Keoughand  Downes,  1982;  Wethey,  1986; 
LeTourneux  and  Bourget,  1988),  or  relative  to  microflora 
(e.g..  Crisp  and  Ryland,  1960;  Brancato  and  Woollacott, 
1982;  Strathmann  et  ai.  1981).  These  patterns  may  arise 

Received  27  July  1990;  accepted  29  November  1990. 


at  the  time  of  larval  settlement  or  develop  later  as  a  result 
of  differential  mortality.  The  distribution  of  individuals 
at  the  time  of  larval  settlement  has  a  strong  influence  on 
their  future  success.  Individuals  that  settle  near  dominant 
competitors  are  more  likely  to  die  quickly,  as  are  those 
that  settle  within  the  range  of  predators  or  where  distur- 
bance events  frequently  occur. 

There  are  a  number  of  potential  escapes  from  sources 
of  biotic  mortality,  including  simple  avoidance  of  settle- 
ment near  enemies  (e.g..  Grosberg,  1981;  Young  and  Chia, 
1981)  and  recruitment  to  spatial  refuges  (e.g.,  Connell, 
1961;  Dayton.  1971;  Paine,  1974;  Wethey,  1983;  Walters 
and  Wethey,  1986).  Organisms  located  in  spatial  refuges 
increase  their  chances  of  survival  against  competitors, 
predators,  and  disturbance  events.  Size  can  also  be  pro- 
tective to  colonies  once  they  have  grown  to  certain  di- 
mensions unaffected  by  competitors;  this  is  the  size  refuge. 
Potential  morphological  escapes  may  also  exist.  Among 
colonial  organisms  attached  to  hard  substrata,  one  can 
distinguish  a  number  of  morphological  types,  including 
sheet  and  tree  forms  (Jackson,  1979).  The  outcomes  of 
competitive  interactions  can  be  strongly  influenced  by  the 
morphologies  of  the  competitors.  Tree  forms  are  relatively 
isolated  from  the  substratum-associated  competitors 
(Jackson,  1979;  Grosberg,  1981),  whereas  sheet  forms  en- 
crust the  substratum  and  may  suffer  competitive  inter- 
actions along  their  edges.  Thin  sheets  tend  to  lose  to 
thicker  forms  (Buss,  1980;  Seed  and  O'Connor,  1981; 
Russ,  1982;  Sebens,  1985,  1986;  Walters  and  Wethey, 
1986)  unless  they  have  a  height  advantage  in  the  zone  of 
contact  (Walters  and  Wethey,  1986).  Therefore,  one  would 
predict  that  animals  with  thin,  sheet-like  growth  forms 
should  preferentially  settle  on  or  near  locations  where  they 
have  a  height  advantage  (Walters  and  Wethey,  1986). 

Although  tree  forms  are  less  likely  to  be  overgrown  by 
competitors,  they  can  be  more  visible  to  predators  and 


112 


INVERTEBRATE  SETTLEMENT  REFUGES 


113 


are  more  susceptible  to  total  colony  mortality  than  sheet 
forms.  On  irregular  substrata,  a  potential  settlement  refuge 
location  would  be  found  around  the  bases  of  bumps.  Here, 
certain  predators  may  not  be  able  to  reach  newly  settled 
individuals.  Here  they  are  also  protected  from  more  dis- 
turbance events  than  they  would  be  if  they  were  located 
on  a  flat  surface  or  on  the  top  of  a  bump. 

We  examined  the  patterns  of  larval  settlement  in  three 
species  of  encrusting  colonial  animals  with  different 
growth  forms.  We  asked  whether  the  settlement  patterns 
were  consistent  with  our  prediction  that  species  with  thin 
sheet  morphologies  should  choose  spatial  refuges  from 
competitors,  whereas  species  with  tree  morphologies 
should  choose  refuge  locations  that  would  reduce  the  risk 
of  predation  and  disturbance.  The  encrusting  cheilostome 
bryozoan  Membranipora  membranacea  was  our  example 
of  a  thin  sheet  morphology,  and  the  arborescent  bryozoan 
Bugiila  neritina  and  the  pedunculate  ascidian  Distaplia 
occidentalis  were  our  examples  of  tree  morphologies.  We 
examined  two  kinds  of  substrata.  The  kelp  Laminaria 
saccharina  is  a  substratum  commonly  colonized  by  all 
three  species.  Settlement  plates  cast  from  bumps  on  Lego 
toy  building  blocks  and  pits  created  from  bubble  plastic 
served  as  model  topographies  of  the  same  spatial  scale  as 
those  found  on  Laminaria.  Our  analysis  was  carried  out 
in  two  phases:  ( 1 )  we  examined  the  extent  to  which  set- 
tlement on  our  model  substrata  mimicked  that  on  natural 
surfaces;  and  (2)  we  examined  in  detail  the  spatial  pattern 
of  settlement  on  the  model  substrata. 

Materials  and  Methods 

Sludy  organisms 

The  bryozoans  Membranipora  membranacea  and 
Bugiila  neritina  have  small,  ciliated  larvae  (Membrani- 
pora: 750  urn;  Bugiila:  200  ^m,  from  Reed,  1987)  that 
have  limited  swimming  abilities  in  the  ocean  (Chia  et  ai, 
1984).  However,  these  larvae  can  choose  their  settlement 
locations.  When  competent,  they  move  closely  over  the 
substrata  and  test  it  (Woollacott  and  Zimmer,  1978,  for 
Bugiila:  Atkins,  1955,  for  Membranipora).  During  this 
phase,  Bugit/a  larvae  form  temporary  attachments  using 
adhesives  that  are  sufficiently  strong  to  prevent  the  indi- 
vidual from  being  mechanically  dislodged  (Loeb  and 
Walker,  1977).  Bugiila  can  quickly  dissolve  the  adhesive 
or  change  its  viscosity  to  detach  from,  or  reject  the  surface 
(Reed  and  Woollacott,  1982). 

In  the  plankton,  Distaplia  occidentalis  larvae  are  much 
larger  than  those  of  the  other  two  species,  measuring  up 
to  3.2  mm  in  length  (Cloney  and  Torrence,  1984).  Most 
encounter  a  number  of  surface  locations  before  meta- 
morphosing on  one  of  them  (R.  A.  Cloney,  pers.  comm.). 
Torrence  and  Cloney  (1988)  suggest  that  sensory  neurons 
in  the  adhesive  papillae  may  be  common  in  ascidians.  In 


the  laboratory,  adhesion  in  Distaplia  occurs  within  30  s 
at  15°C  (Cloney,  1978).  Tail  resorption  reduces  the  size 
of  the  newly  settled  individual  to  approximately  650  ^m 
within  7  min  (Cloney,  1978). 

For  the  purposes  of  this  study,  it  was  important  to  dis- 
tinguish between  newly  settled  and  metamorphosed  in- 
dividuals. Newly  metamorphosed  Membranipora  colonies 
have  only  the  twin  ancestrula  skeleton  fully  formed,  and 
Bugiila  has  only  the  first  zooid  skeleton  completed.  Dis- 
laplia  colonies  were  considered  new  individuals  if  they 
occupied  less  than  1  mm2. 

Experimental  procedure 

To  study  larval  settlement  on  natural  substrata,  we  ex- 
amined the  alga  Laminaria  saccharina.  Plants  were  col- 
lected on  the  floating  docks  at  the  Friday  Harbor  Labo- 
ratories, San  Juan  Island,  Washington  state  (48°  32'  42" 
N;  123°  0'  39"  W)  and  on  the  floating  public  docks  at 
Fisherman's  Bay  on  Lopez  Island,  Washington  state  (48° 
30'30"N;  122°  54'51"W).  Entire  blades  were  either  placed 
in  running  seawater  tables  and  a  census  taken  within  48 
h,  or  frozen  immediately  for  a  later  census.  Random  pieces 
of  the  alga  (20  X  20  cm)  were  cut  from  the  central  portion 
of  large  ( 1 .0-2.0  m  in  length)  Laminaria  fronds.  All  new 
settlers  were  recorded  on  each  algal  square.  As  the  topog- 
raphies of  the  blades  are  quite  variable,  we  could  not  dis- 
tinguish a  pit  from  a  bump.  Instead,  each  topographical 
feature  on  the  blade  was  defined  as  a  continuous  slope 
extending  from  a  lowest  to  a  highest  point  (Fig.  1).  The 
lowest  point  on  one  side  of  an  algal  blade  is  the  highest 
point  on  the  reverse  side.  The  diameter  (base)  and  the 
height  of  each  topographic  feature  were  recorded  with 
vernier  calipers.  The  slopes  ranged  in  length  from  1  to  20 
mm.  The  location  of  each  animal  was  determined  by  cre- 


Hiqhest   point 


Height  of   topogrophic 
feoture   (h) 


Distonce  from   lowest   point  to   animal 


Diameter  of  topographic   feature   (d) 

Figure  I.  Each  colony  was  mapped  in  relation  to  the  nearest  topo- 
graphic high  and  low  point.  The  dimensions  of  the  topographic  feature 
were  measured. 


14 


L    J.  WALTERS  AND  D.  S.  WETHEY 


ating  a  right  triangle  with  the  animal  location  and  the 
lowest  point  as  two  of  the  points  (Fig.  1 ).  The  distance 
from  the  lowest  point  to  the  animal  and  the  animal  height 
above  the  lowest  point  were  measured  (Fig.  1 ). 

Using  the  diameter  (d)  and  height  (h)  of  each  topo- 
graphic feature,  we  calculated: 
( 1 )  the  radius  of  curvature  (re)  of  the  topographic  feature: 


re  = 


h2  +  (d/2) 


2*(1  +  d)*(d/(2*h)) 

(2)  the  vertical  position  (vp)  of  the  animal,  which  we  use 
to  determine  the  location  (top,  side  or  base)  of  the  organ- 
ism on  the  topographic  feature: 

vp  =  (animal  height/h). 

Wilcoxon  rank  sum  tests  were  used  to  determine  if  there 
were  differences  in  locations  occupied  by  larvae  of  the 
three  common  species.  We  examined  the  effects  of  size 
and  shape  of  topographic  features  (height,  diameter,  and 
radius  of  curvature)  as  well  as  larval  position  (animal 
height  and  vertical  position).  When  differences  were 
found,  pairwise  Wilcoxon  rank  sum  tests  were  run  to  de- 
termine which  species  were  significantly  different.  Data 
from  the  Friday  Harbor  Laboratories  and  Lopez  Island 
were  pooled  after  Wilcoxon  rank  sum  tests  showed  that 
there  were  no  differences  between  the  two  sites. 

To  model  the  kinds  and  size  scales  of  topographic  fea- 
tures found  on  natural  substrata,  such  as  the  alga  Lami- 
naria  saccharina,  we  constructed  three  types  of  plastic 
plates  8.9  cm  in  diameter:  (1)  small  Lego  (Lego  Systems 
Inc.)  building  block  bumps  (cylindrical,  2  mm  high,  5 
mm  diameter)  simulated  small  algal  bumps;  (2)  large  Lego 
building  block  bumps  (cylindrical,  5  mm  high,  9  mm  di- 
ameter) simulated  large  algal  bumps;  and  (3)  bubble  plastic 
pits  (hemispherical,  2  mm  deep,  5  mm  diameter)  simu- 
lated small  algal  pits.  These  materials  were  used  because 
their  topographic  features  were  of  the  appropriate  spatial 
scale  and  were  uniformly  spaced.  We  produced  settlement 
plates  by  pouring  polyester  resin  into  silicone  rubber 
molds  (Sylgard  184  Silicone  Elastomer,  Dow  Corning 
Corp.).  Black  resin  pigment  (Titan  Corp.)  was  added  to 
the  uncatalyzed  resin  to  make  newly  settled  larvae  more 
visible  on  the  plates. 

The  settlement  plates  were  attached  to  wooden  boards 
with  stainless  steel  screws.  These  were  hung  beneath  the 
floating  docks  with  polypropylene  rope.  The  plates  were 
oriented  face  down  to  prevent  algal  colonization.  Six  rep- 
licates of  each  surface  were  submerged  in  each  trial.  Plates 
were  arranged  in  a  Latin  square  design,  with  one  replicate 
of  each  type  of  plate  on  each  board.  Six  trials  were  run 
during  the  summers  of  1987  and  1989. 

Photographs  were  taken  every  two  days  at  the  Friday 
Harbor  Laboratories  and  once  or  twice  a  week  at  Lopez 
Island  during  1987.  Additional  data  were  collected  by  di- 


rect observation  at  Lopez  Island  in  1989.  Flash-lit  pho- 
tographs were  taken  underwater  using  Kodak  Technical 
Pan  2415  film  and  a  Nikonos  5  camera  equipped  with  a 
5:1  extension  tube  and  focal  framer.  Negatives  were  ob- 
served under  a  dissecting  microscope  equipped  with  an 
ocular  micrometer  to  determine  the  specific  locations  of 
newly  settled  individuals.  We  distinguished  among  four 
kinds  of  locations  on  the  plates  with  bumps:  (1)  top  of 
bump;  (2)  side  of  bump;  (3)  touching  the  base  of  the  bump; 
and  (4)  on  the  flat  surface  not  touching  the  base  of  the 
bump.  On  the  pitted  surface,  we  distinguished  among 
three  kinds  of  locations:  ( 1 )  in  the  pit;  (2)  touching  the 
edge  of  the  pit,  and  (3)  on  the  flat  surface  not  touching 
the  pit.  Individual  larvae  were  scored  as  touching  a  to- 
pographic feature  if  they  were  within  250  ^m  of  the  fea- 
ture. This  distance  represents  approximately  one  body 
length  of  the  settled  larvae  (200  to  750  urn  in  length). 

To  determine  whether  larvae  settled  preferentially  in 
relation  to  topographic  features,  we  compared  our  obser- 
vations to  a  random  distribution.  For  example,  if  larvae 
settled  randomly,  then  the  proportion  of  larvae  settling 
in  pits  should  be  equal  to  the  proportion  of  space  ac- 
counted for  by  pits.  In  this  way  we  calculated  the  number 
of  larvae  expected  to  settle  in  each  of  our  classes  of  lo- 
cations (on  or  in  pits  or  bumps,  touching  pits  or  bumps, 
away  from  pits  or  bumps).  Paired  simultaneous  /-tests 
were  used  to  compare  the  observed  versus  expected  num- 
ber of  individuals  in  each  location  on  a  settlement  plate. 
The  simultaneous  /-tests  were  weighted  because  the  esti- 
mates of  proportions  of  larvae  were  all  based  on  samples 
of  different  sizes.  The  estimate  p  of  a  proportion  has  a 
gaussian  distribution  with  a  variance  p(  1  -  p)/N,  where 
N  is  the  sample  size  (Snedecor  and  Cochran,  1967:  p. 
208).  We  weighted  our  estimates  by  the  reciprocal  of  this 
variance  because  we  have  higher  confidence  in  estimates 
with  the  lowest  variance.  Plates  with  less  than  two  indi- 
viduals were  not  included.  We  used  the  Bonferroni  in- 
equality to  make  the  tests  simultaneous  (Miller,  1966). 
For  example,  when  we  compared  three  settlement  loca- 
tions, to  maintain  an  overall  error  rate  of  0.05,  we  used 
an  error  rate  of  0.05/3  =  0.016  in  each  individual  com- 
parison. 

To  determine  whether  settlement  preference  changed 
as  space  became  occupied,  we  examined  the  relationship 
between  the  proportion  of  larvae  settling  in  the  feature 
and  the  proportion  of  unoccupied  space  accounted  for  by 
that  feature.  On  all  dates  we  calculated  the  space  available 
for  settlement  by  subtracting  from  the  total  the  area  oc- 
cupied by  settled  individuals.  We  assumed  that  all  newly 
metamorphosed  larvae  occupied  1  mm2.  We  compared 
settlement  in  samples  with  more  than  the  average  amount 
of  free  space,  to  settlement  in  samples  with  less  than  the 
average  amount  of  free  space. 


INVERTEBRATE  SETTLEMENT  REFUGES 


115 


Stoloniferous  hydrozoan  (primarily  Obelia  dichotoma 
and  Obelia  geniculata)  and  entoproct  (Barentxia  hcncdcni) 
colonies  were  present  on  all  of  the  plates  within  10  days, 
and  at  least  a  few  stolons  rapidly  covered  the  entire  surface 
of  most  plates.  To  determine  whether  the  stolons  affected 
settlement  of  Bugula,  Distaplia,  and  Membranipora,  the 
tops  of  the  Lego  bumps  were  divided  into  ten  pie-shaped 
wedges.  Similarly,  the  bases  of  the  Lego  bumps  were  di- 
vided into  ten  equal  sections.  If  settlement  was  random 
with  respect  to  stolons,  then  the  ratio  of  wedges  where 
stolons  and  larvae  co-occur,  to  wedges  with  larvae,  should 
equal  the  ratio  of  wedges  with  stolons  to  total  wedges. 
Paired  simultaneous  /-tests  were  used  to  determine 
whether  the  observed  and  expected  ratios  were  equal. 

Very  few  individuals  of  other  species  settled  on  our 
experimental  plates.  Approximately  75%  of  the  plates  of 
each  type  had  no  other  species  settling  on  them.  The  re- 
maining 25%  had  an  average  of  two  individuals  of  other 
species  on  them.  These  other  species  included:  the  bryo- 
zoans  Tegella  armifera  and  Schizoporella  itnicornis,  the 
ascidian  Diplosoma  macdonaldi,  the  barnacle  Balanus 
crenatus,  the  serpulid  polychaete  worm  Pseudochitono- 
ponui  occidentalis,  and  spirorbid  polychaetes. 


Results 


Natural  alga  substrata 


Bugula  nentina,  Distaplia  occidentalis,  and  Membran- 
ipora membranacea  settled  in  locations  with  similar  di- 
ameters and  radii  of  curvature  (Table  I).  Bugula  and  Dis- 
taplia settled  in  significantly  lower  elevations  relative  to 
topographic  features  than  did  Membranipora  (Table  I: 
Vertical  Position).  Bugula  settled  on  topographic  features 
that  were  significantly  taller  than  those  on  which  the  other 
two  species  settled  (Table  I). 

Settlement  plate  experiments 

On  the  Lego  settlement  plates,  settlement  was  non-ran- 
dom for  all  species  (Table  II).  Distaplia  and  Bugula  were 
found  most  often  around  the  bases  of  bumps  (Table  II). 
These  locations  covered  less  than  5%  of  the  total  surface 
area  of  the  settlement  plates,  yet  more  than  50%  of  the 
larvae  of  Distaplia  and  Bugula  settled  there. 

Both  arborescent  forms,  Distaplia  and  Bugula,  were 
found  significantly  less  often  than  expected  on  flat  surfaces 
of  the  large  and  small  Legos  and  the  flat  surfaces  of  plates 
with  small  pits  (Table  II).  Distaplia  settled  more  than  ex- 
pected by  chance  in  the  pits.  In  contrast,  Bugula  signifi- 
cantly avoided  pits  (Table  II).  The  sheet  form,  Membran- 
ipora, was  found  more  than  expected  on  the  tops  of  bumps 
and  on  the  flat  surfaces  away  from  the  topographic  features 
in  the  large  Lego  treatment,  but  less  than  expected  around 
the  bases  of  bumps  (Table  II).  On  the  pitted  surfaces. 


Table  I 

Settlement  location*  o/Membranipora  membranacea.  Bugula  neritina 
and  Distaplia  occidentalis  mi  the  alt;a  Laminaria  sacchanna 


Species 


Mean 


Group 


Height  of  Topographic  Feature 

Bugula 

64                          9.22 

A 

Distaplia 

95                            7.  XI 

B 

Membranipora 

147                          7.80 

B 

Bugula 

Distaplia 

Mcmhranipora 


Diameter  of  Topographic  Feature 
64 


147 


26.30 
28.13 
27.12 


Radius  of.  Curvature  of  the  Topographic  Feature 


Bugula 

Dislaplia 

Membranipora 


Biiguta 

Distaplia 

Membranipora 


Bugula 

Distaplia 

Membranipora 


64 
95 
147 


213.62 
446.45 
453.29 


Animal  Height  Above  Lowest  Point 


64 
95 
147 


1.80 

1.44 
5.32 


Vertical  Position  of  the  Animal 


64 

95 

147 


0.23 
0.18 

0.71 


A 
A 
A 


A 
A 
A 


A 
A 
B 


A 
A 
B 


N  =  the  number  of  individuals.  Mean  =  the  mean  in  millimeters,  and 
Group  =  the  results  of  Wilcoxon  rank  sum  tests.  Different  letters  refer 
to  significant  differences  (P  <  0.05).  For  explanation  of  the  measured 
values,  see  Figure  1  and  the  text. 

Membranipora  settled  significantly  less  than  expected  in 
the  pits  and  more  than  expected  around  the  edges  of  the 
pits.  Bugula  and  Distaplia  settled  preferentially  around 
the  bases  of  bumps,  while  Membranipora  appeared  to 
avoid  this  location.  To  estimate  whether  there  was  pre- 
emption of  space  by  Bugula  and  Distaplia,  we  compared 
Membranipora  settlement  in  samples  with  more  than  the 
average  percent  free  space  to  settlement  in  samples  with 
less  than  the  average.  Free  space  around  the  bumps  de- 
creased during  the  settlement  season  from  2.0%  to  1.5% 
on  the  small  Legos  and  from  4.3%  to  3.6%  on  large  Legos. 
Membranipora  settlement  was  independent  of  availability 
of  free  space  on  both  large  Lego  plates  (F  =  0.24;  d.f.  =  1, 
23:  P  =  0.63).  and  small  Lego  plates  (F  =  0.16;  d.f.  =  1, 
19;  P  =  0.69). 

The  settling  larvae  were  not  affected  by  the  presence  of 
hydrozoan  or  entoproct  stolons  (Table  III).  The  larvae 
neither  preferentially  settled  in  locations  where  stolons 
were  present  nor  did  they  significantly  avoid  these  loca- 
tions. 


116 


L.  J.  WALTERS  AND  D.  S.  WETHEY 


Table  II 

Test  oi f  randomness  of  settlement  locations:  the  results  of  simultaneous 
paired  l-lests  comparing  the  i:\peclcel  versus 
the  observed  number  ot  settlers 


Species 

Location 

N         Difference 

S.E. 

Sign. 

Large  Lego 

Bugula 

Top 

16 

-2.14 

0.33 

Less 

Base 

16 

5.28 

0.64 

More 

Flat 

16 

-2.60 

0.96 

Less 

Di.itaplia 

Top 

25 

-2.60 

0.36 

Less 

Base 

25 

11.05 

1.33 

More 

Flat 

25 

1.18 

0.46 

Less 

Membranipora 

Top 

16 

5.47 

1.16 

More 

Base 

16 

-0.65 

0.  1  3 

Less 

Rat 

16 

3.92 

0.57 

More 

Small  Lego 

Bugula 

Top 

16 

-2.75 

0.43 

Less 

Base 

16 

7.01 

1.30 

More 

Flat 

16 

-2.57 

0.77 

Less 

Distaplia 

Top 

25 

-4.05 

0.49 

Less 

Base 

25 

15.78 

1.99 

More 

Rat 

25 

-5.67 

0.90 

Less 

Membranipora 

Top 

15 

1.21 

0.88 

n.s. 

Base 

15 

-0.20 

0.08 

n.s. 

Rat 

15 

1.26 

0.84 

n.s. 

Small  Pits 

Bugula 

Pit 

1  1 

-1.86 

0.60 

Less 

Edge 

11 

3.22 

0.96 

More 

Flat 

1  1 

-1.51 

0.43 

Less 

Distaplia 

Pit 

19 

3.99 

0.82 

More 

Edge 

19 

1.38 

0.29 

More 

Flat 

19 

-4.57 

0.85 

Less 

Membranipora 

Pit 

9 

-3.23 

0.78 

Less 

Edge 

9 

1.64 

0.52 

More 

Flat 

9 

2.15 

0.95 

n.s. 

N  =  the  number  ot  plates  on  which  at  least  two  larvae  settled;  Difference 
=  the  mean  for  N  plates  of  the  observed  -  expected  values;  S.E.  =  the 
standard  error  of  the  Difference;  and  Sign.  =  the  direction  of  the  signif- 
icance value  with  n.s.  =  not  significant  (P  >  0.05).  A  Bonferonni  com- 
parisonwise  error  rate  of  0.016  was  used  to  keep  the  expenmentwise 
error  rate  =  0.05. 


Discussion 

In  this  study  we  examined  the  relationship  between 
larval  settlement  pattern  and  adult  growth  form  in  colonial 
epifauna  on  hard  substrata.  We  asked  whether  larvae  of 
species  with  thin  sheet  morphologies  chose  different  set- 
tlement locations  from  those  of  larvae  of  species  with  ar- 
borescent morphologies.  We  argued  that  species  with  thin 
sheet  growth  forms  should  be  more  susceptible  to  over- 
growth by  competitors  than  species  with  tree  morpholo- 
gies. Because  topographic  high  spots  may  serve  as  spatial 


refuges  from  competitors  (Walters  and  Wethey  1986),  we 
expected  species  with  thin  sheet  morphologies  to  settle 
preferentially  on  topographic  high  spots. 

In  the  present  study,  the  thin  sheet  species,  Membran- 
ipora membranacea.  preferentially  settled  on  the  highest 
available  locations  on  topographically  complex  surfaces 
(tops  of  bumps  and  flat  areas  between  pits:  Table  II).  This 
is  consistent  with  our  predictions,  because  the  tops  of 
bumps  and  the  flat  areas  on  a  pitted  surface  are  both  lo- 
cations where  a  colony  has  a  height  advantage  over  com- 
petitors, and  thus  has  a  potential  refuge  from  competition. 
This  result  indicates  that  physical  cues  may  allow  larvae 
to  escape  from  competitors,  much  as  biogenic  cues  (e.g., 
Grosberg,  1981;  Young  and  Chia,  1981)  allow  larvae  to 
avoid  recruitment  near  enemies. 

We  argued  that  species  with  arborescent  growth  forms 
should  be  relatively  immune  to  competitors,  but  that  they 
might  suffer  damage  from  mobile  predators  like  fish.  In 
North  Carolina,  for  example,  filefish  feed  voraciously  on 
newly  settled  colonies  of  Bugula  stolonifera  growing  on 
flat  surfaces  (L.J.W.,  pers.  obs.).  Thus,  tree  forms  might 
be  expected  to  settle  in  cracks  and  crevices.  In  the  present 
study,  the  arborescent  forms,  Distaplia  occidentalis  and 
Bugula  neritina.  settled  preferentially  around  the  bases  of 
topographic  irregularities  (Table  II).  This  result  is  consis- 
tent with  our  predictions,  because  the  bases  of  bumps  on 
our  experimental  plates  are  the  locations  most  like  crev- 
ices. 

Similar  spatial  partitioning  occurred  on  the  alga  Lam- 
inaria  saccharina  (Table  I).  Arborescent  Distaplia  and 
Bugula  were  found  low  on  the  slopes  of  the  alga,  while 
the  thin  sheet  Membranipora  was  found  significantly 
higher  (Table  I:  Animal  Height).  The  algal  low  spots. 


Table  III 
Test  of  response  of  larvae  to  hydroid  and  enioproct  stolons 


Species 

Location 

N 

Diff. 

S.E. 

P 
Value 

Sign. 

Bugula 

Top 

1 

0.00 

N.A. 

N.A. 

N.A. 

Bugula 

Base 

22 

-0.53 

0.50 

0.3293 

n.s. 

Distaplia 

Top 

4 

2.50 

2.50 

0.3910 

n.s. 

Distaplia 

Base 

43 

-1.10 

3.11 

0.7243 

n.s. 

Membranipora 

Top 

50 

3.25 

2.82 

0.2553 

n.s. 

Membranipora 

Base 

10 

-2.14 

8.86 

0.8144 

n.s. 

If  settlement  is  random  with  respect  to  stolons,  then  the  ratio  of  wedges 
with  stolons  and  larvae,  to  wedges  with  larvae,  should  be  equal  to  the 
ratio  of  wedges  with  stolons  to  total  wedges.  N  =  the  number  of  indi- 
viduals; Diff.  =  the  mean  of  the  difference:  [(wedges  with  stolons)/!  total 
wedges)]  -  [(wedges  with  larvae  +  stolons)/! wedges  with  larvae)];  S.E. 
=  the  standard  error  of  the  Difference;  and  Sign.  =  the  sign  of  the  sig- 
nificance value  if  a  <  0.05.  A  negative  difference  denotes  bumps  that 
had  more  larvae  settling  than  it  had  stolons,  and  N.A.  =  not  applicable. 


INVERTEBRATE  SETTLEMENT  REFUGES 


17 


where  Bitgula  and  Distaplia  settled,  are  functionally 
equivalent  to  the  bases  of  Lego  bumps  and  the  pits  in  the 
artificial  settlement  surfaces  (Table  II).  Similarly,  the  high 
positions  on  algal  slopes  where  Membranipora  settled  are 
functionally  equivalent  to  the  elevated  locations  where 
they  settled  on  the  settlement  plates  (Table  II).  However, 
topography  does  not  fully  control  settlement  pattern,  be- 
cause neither  previously  settled  individuals,  nor  the  stolon 
mats  of  hydrozoans  and  entoprocts,  affected  settlement 
by  the  larvae  (Table  III),  even  though  the  presence  of  any 
organisms  on  the  substratum  alters  the  local  microtopog- 
raphy. 

An  alternative  mechanism  that  could  account  for  the 
settlement  patterns  is  passive  transport  of  larvae  by  hy- 
drodynamic  forces.  Because  of  their  limited  swimming 
abilities  (Chia  el  ai,  1981).  larvae  are  often  passively 
transported  in  boundary  layer  flows  (e.g.,  Butman,  1987). 
One  can  model  passive  larval  transport  as  analogous  to 
sediment  transport  (e.g.,  Middleton  and  Southward, 
1984).  The  patterns  of  transport  are  influenced  by  the 
turbulent  motion  of  the  water  and  by  the  topography  of 
the  substratum.  When  the  surface  topography  protrudes 
beyond  the  'viscous  sublayer'  into  the  turbulent  overlying 
water,  turbulent  eddies  can  cause  erosion.  The  roughness 
Reynolds  number.  Re*,  is  a  measure  of  the  degree  to 
which  roughness  elements  protrude  above  the  viscous 
sublayer: 

Re*  =  u*Lp/M 

where  u*  is  the  shear  velocity  of  the  fluid  flow  regime,  L 
is  the  height  of  the  roughness  element,  p  is  the  density  of 
seawater,  and  fj.  is  the  dynamic  viscosity  of  seawater. 

In  a  wave-influenced  environment,  u*  is  approximately 
10%  of  the  maximum  water  velocity  (Denny,  1988;  Denny 
and  Shibata,  1989;  Svenden,  1987).  We  estimate  u*  to  be 
in  the  range  of  1.6  to  2.4  cm/s,  yielding  Re*  values  of 
30-50  for  the  small  Legos  and  75-120  for  the  large  Legos. 
If  the  roughness  Reynolds  number  is  less  than  5,  the 
bumps  lie  within  the  viscous  sublayer.  Thus,  in  all  cases, 
the  bumps  on  our  settlement  plates  are  in  a  potentially 
erosional  regime.  Larvae  differ  from  sediment  particles  in 
their  ability  to  adhere  to  surfaces.  In  tlume  experiments 
with  our  settlement  plates,  sediment  never  accumulated 
on  the  tops  of  the  Lego  bumps,  presumably  because  the 
erosional  forces  are  very  high  in  these  locations.  Therefore, 
if  the  pattern  were  passive,  larvae  would  not  have  accu- 
mulated on  the  tops  of  bumps.  However,  the  tops  of  the 
bumps  are  the  locations  where  Membranipora  larvae  did 
accumulate.  Thus,  we  believe  that  the  passive  model  can- 
not explain  our  patterns. 

Competitive  interactions  were  infrequent  on  these  set- 
tlement plates,  because  recruitment  rates  were  low  and 
space  did  not  become  limiting  during  our  experiments. 
The  only  common  encounters  were  between  the  entoproct 


and  hydrozoan  stolons  and  the  three  species,  with  the 
later  arrival  always  growing  over  the  previously  established 
colony.  Neither  colony  appeared  to  be  affected  by  these 
interactions.  Although  space  was  not  filled  on  our  settle- 
ment plates  during  the  time  course  of  this  study,  little 
bare  space  existed  on  the  docks  from  which  the  plates 
were  suspended.  Because  so  little  free  space  existed  on 
the  persistent  hard  substrata,  we  believe  that  competition 
could  act  as  a  selective  agent  on  larval  behavior. 

The  results  of  these  studies  are  consistent  with  our  pre- 
diction that  adult  body  form  should  be  correlated  with 
larval  settlement  pattern.  The  arborescent  forms  (Bugnla 
and  Distaplia)  settled  preferentially  in  the  small  amount 
of  space  touching  the  bases  of  the  bumps,  potentially  hid- 
den from  predators  and  disturbance  events.  The  thin  sheet 
form  (Alembranipora)  settled  most  frequently  on  the 
highest  available  locations  on  topographically  complex 
surfaces.  Thus  Membranipora,  the  adult  growth  form  of 
which  is  most  susceptible  to  overgrowth,  had  larvae  that 
settled  in  potential  refuges  from  competitors.  Adult  com- 
petitive ability  and  susceptibility  to  predation  and  distur- 
bance may  be  an  important  influence  on  selection  for 
larval  settlement  behavior. 

Acknowledgments 

This  study  was  supported  by  the  University  of  South 
Carolina,  grants  from  the  Office  of  Naval  Research  (Con- 
tract N00014-82-K-0645)  and  the  National  Science 
Foundation  (Grant  OCE86-00531)  to  D.  Wethey  and 
grants  from  the  Lerner-Gray  Fund  for  Marine  Research, 
Sigma  Xi,  and  the  International  Women's  Fishing  As- 
sociation to  L.  Walters.  We  are  grateful  to  all  at  the  Friday 
Harbor  Laboratories  for  providing  us  with  space  and  fa- 
cilities. D.  Padilla,  D.  Pencheff,  L.  Muehlstein,  A.  Kettle, 
S.  Cohen,  A.  Sewell,  and  countless  others  assisted  with 
the  field  work.  J.  Sutherland  and  A.  Underwood  assisted 
with  the  statistical  analyses.  S.  Woodin,  J.  Sutherland,  R. 
Showman,  and  two  anonymous  reviewers  made  helpful 
comments  on  the  manuscript. 

Literature  Cited 

Atkins,  1955.  The  cyphonautes  larvae  of  the  Plymouth  Area  and  the 
metamorphosis  of  Membranipora  membranacea.  J.  Mar.  Biol.  Axsiv. 
L'  K  34:441-449. 

Brancato,  M.  S.,  and  R.  S.  VVoollacott.  1982.  Effect  of  microbial  films 
on  settlement  of  bryozoan  larvae  (Bugida  simplex.  B.  slolonifera  and 
B  n/rriui).  .Mar,  Bml.  71:  51-56. 

Buss,  L.  W.  1980.  Bryozoan  overgrowth  interactions — the  interde- 
pendence of  competition  for  space  and  food.  Nature  281:  475-477. 

Butman,  C.  A.  1987.  Larval  settlement  of  soft-sediment  invertebrates: 
the  spatial  scales  of  pattern  explained  by  active  habitat  selection  and 
the  emerging  role  of  hydrodynamic  processes.  Oceanogr.  Mar  Biol 
Ann.  Rev.  25:  113-165. 

Chia,  F-S.,  J.  Buckland-Nicks,  and  C.  M.  Young.  1984.  Locomotion 
of  marine  invertebrate  larvae:  a  review.  Can.  J  Zoo/.  62:  1205-1222. 


118 


L.  J.  WALTERS  AND  D.  S.  WETHEY 


Clone),  R.  A.  1978.  Ascidian  metamorphosis:  review  and  analysis.  Pp. 
255-282  in  Settlement  ami  Metamorphosis  <>l  Mamie  Invertebrate 
Larvae,  F-S.  Chia  and  M.  E.  Rice.  eds.  Elsevier.  New  York. 

Cloney,  R.  A.,  and  S.  A.  Torrence.  1984.  Ascidian  larvae:  structure 
and  settlement.  Pp.  103-126  in  Marine Biodeterioration,  J.  D.  Costlow 
and  R.  C.  Tipper,  eds.  Naval  Institute  Press.  Annapolis. 

Connell,  J.  H.  1961.  The  effects  of  competition,  predation  by  Than 
lapillus.  and  other  factors  on  natural  populations  of  the  barnacle, 
Balanus  balanoides.  Ecol.  Monogr  31:  61-104. 

Crisp,  D.  J.  1961.  Territorial  behavior  in  barnacle  settlement.  J  Exp 
Biol  38:  569-590. 

Crisp,  D.  J.,  and  H.  Barnes.  1954.  The  orientation  and  distribution  of 
barnacles  at  settlement  with  particular  reference  to  surface  contour. 
J.  Anim.  Eco/.  23:  142-162. 

Crisp,  D.  J.,  and  J.S.  Ryland.  I960.  Influence  of  filming  and  of  surface 
texture  on  the  settlement  of  marine  organisms.  Xalnre  185:  1 19. 

Dayton,  P.  1971.  Competition,  disturbance,  and  community  organi- 
zation: the  provision  and  subsequent  utilization  of  space  in  the  rocky 
intertidal  community.  Ecol.  Monogr.  41:  351-389. 

Denny,  M.  \V.  1988.  Biology  and  Mechanics  of  the  Have-Swept  En- 
vironment Princeton  University  Press,  Princeton,  NJ. 

Denny,  M.  VV.,  and  M.  F.  Shibata.  1989.  Consequences  of  surf-zone 
turbulence  for  settlement  and  external  fertilization.  Am.  .\al.  134: 
859-889. 

Grosberg,  R.  K.  1981.  Competitive  ability  influences  habitat  choice  in 
marine  environments.  Nature  290:  700-702. 

Hayward,  P.  J.,  and  P.  H.  Harvey.  1974.  The  distribution  of  settled 
larvae  of  the  bryozoans  Alcynidium  hirsutum  (Fleming)  and  Alcyn- 
idium  polyoum  (Hassall).  J  Mar  Bio/.  Assoc  I'  K  54:  665-676. 

Jackson,  J.  B.  C.  1979.  Morphological  strategies  of  sessile  animals. 
Pp.  499-555  in  Biology  and  Systematics  of  Colonial  Organisms,  G. 
Larwood  and  B.  R.  Rosen,  eds.  Academic  Press,  New  York. 

keough,  M.  J.,  and  B.  J.  Downes.  1982.  Recruitment  of  marine  in- 
vertebrates: the  role  of  active  larval  choices  and  early  mortality.  Oec- 
ologia  54:  348-352. 

Knight-Jones.  E.  VV.  1951.  Gregariousness  and  some  other  aspects  of 
the  settling  behavior  of  Spirorbis.  J.  Mar.  Biol.  Assoc.  U.  A".  30:  201- 
222. 

LeTourneux,  F.,  and  E.  Bourget.  1988.  Importance  of  physical  and 
biological  settlement  cues  used  at  different  spatial  scales  by  the  larvae 
of  Semibalanus  balanoides.  Mar.  Biol.  97:  57-66. 

Loeb,  M.  J.,  and  G.  Walker.  1977.  Origin,  composition  and  function 
of  secretions  from  pynform  organs  and  internal  sacs  of  four  settling 
cheilo-ctenostome  bryozoan  larvae.  Mar.  Biol.  42:  37-46. 

Middletnn,  G.  \ '.,  and  J.  B.  Southward.  1984.  Mechanics  of  Sediment 
Movement.  2d  ed.  Society  of  Economic  Paleontologists  and  Miner- 
alogists. Tulsa,  Okla. 

Miller,  R.  G.  1966.  Simultaneous  Statistical  Inference.  McGraw-Hill. 
Inc.,  New  York. 

Paine,  R.  T.  1974.  Intertidal  community  structure:  experimental  studies 
on  the  relationship  between  a  dominant  competitor  and  its  principal 
predator.  Oeailogia  15:  93-120. 


Reed,  C.  G.  1987.     Phylum  Bryozoa.  Pp.  494-5 10  in  Reproduction  and 

Development  ol  Marine  Invertebrates  ot  the  Northern  Pacific  (.'oast. 

M.  F.  Strathmann.  ed.  University  of  Washington  Press,  Seattle. 
Reed.  C.  G.,  and  R.  M.  \\oollacott.  1982.     Mechanisms  of  rapid  mor- 

phogenetic  movements  in  the  metamorphosis  of  the  bryozoan  Bugiila 

neritina.  J  Morphol.  172:  335-348. 
Ryland,  J.  S.  1959.     Experiments  on  the  selection  of  algal  substrates  by 

polyzoan  larvae.  J  E.\p.  Biol.  36:  613-631. 
Russ,  G.  R.  1982.     Overgrowth  in  a  marine  epifaunal  community: 

competitive  hierarchies  and  competitive  networks.  Oecologia:  53: 

12-19. 
Sebens,  K.  P.  1985.     Community  ecology  of  vertical  rock  walls  in  the 

Gulf  of  Maine,  U.S.A.:  small  scale  processes  and  alternative  stable 

states.  Pp.  346-371  in  The  Ecology  oj  Rocky  Coasts,  P.  G.  Moore 

and  R.  Seed.  eds.  Hodder  and  Stroughton,  London. 
Sebens,  K.  P.  1986.     Spatial  relationships  among  encrusting  marine 

organisms  in  the  New  England  subtidal  zone.  Ecol.  Monogr.  56:  73- 

96. 
Seed.  R..  and  R.  J.  O'Connor.  1981.     Community  organization  in  marine 

algal  epifaunas.  Ann.  Rev  Ecol  Sysl.  12:  49-74. 
Snedecor,  G.  VV.,  and  VV.  G.  Cochran.  1967.     Statistical  Methods.  Iowa 

State  University  Press.  Ames.  Iowa. 
Strathmann,  R.  R..  K.S.  Branscomb,  and  K.  V'edder.  1981.     Fatal  errors 

in  set  as  a  cost  of  dispersal  and  the  influence  of  intertidal  flora  on  set 

of  barnacles.  Oecologia  48:  13-18. 
Svenden,  I.  A.  1987.     Analysis  of  surf  zone  turbulence.  J.  Geophys.  Res 

92:  51  15-5124. 
Torrence,  S.  A.,  and  R.  A.  Cloney.  1988.     Larval  sensory  organs  of  as- 

cidians.  Pp.  151-163  in  Marine  Biodeterioration,  Advanced  Tech- 
niques Applicable  to  the  Indian  Ocean,  M-F.  Thompson.  R.  Sarogini. 

and  R.  Nagabhushanam,  eds.  Oxford  and  IBH  Publishing  Company 

Pvt.  Ltd..  New  Dehli. 
Walters,  I.. .]..  and  D.  S.  Wethey.  1986.     Surface  topography  influences 

competitive  hierarchies  on  marine  hard  substrata:  a  field  experiment. 

Biol.  Bull.  170:441-449. 
VV  ethey,  D.  S.  1983.     Geographic  limits  and  local  zonation:  the  barnacles 

Semibalanus  (Balanusl  and  Chthamalus  in  New  England.  Biol.  Bull. 

165:  330-341. 
VV  ethe>,  D.  S.  1984.     Spatial  pattern  in  barnacle  settlement:  day  to  day 

changes  during  the  settlement  season.  /  Mar.  Biol.  Assoc  (    A  64: 

687-698. 
Wethey,  D.  S.  1986.     Ranking  of  settlement  cues  by  barnacle  larvae: 

influence  of  surface  contour.  Bull  Mar.  Sci.  39:  393-400. 
Wisely,  B.  1960.     Observations  on  the  settling  behavior  oflarvae  of  the 

tubeworm  Spirorbis  borealts  Daudlin  (Polychaete).  Atixt.  J.  Mar. 

Freshwat.  Res.  II:  55. 
Woollacott,  R.  M.,  and  R.  L.  Zimmer.  1978.     Metamorphosis  of  cel- 

lularioid  bryozoans.  Pp.  49-64  in  Settlement  and  Metamorphosis  of 

Marine  Invertebrate  Larvae.  F-S.  Chia  and  M.  E.  Rice,  eds.  Elsevier. 

New  York. 
Young,  C.  M.,  and  F.  S.  Chia.  1981.     Laboratory  evidence  for  delay  of 

larval  settlement  in  response  to  a  dominant  competitor.  Int.  J  Invert. 

Reprod  3:221-226. 


Reference:  Biol.  Bull  180:  114-124.  (February.  1991) 


GABA-Like  Immunoreactivity  in  the  Nervous  System 
of  Oikopleura  dioica  (Appendicularia) 

TOMAS  BOLLNER'  *.  JON  STORM-MATHISEN2.  AND  OLE  FETTER  OTTERSEN: 

*  Department  of  Zoology.  Stockholm  University,  5-706  91.  Sweden,  and  Anatomical  Institute. 

University  of  Oslo,  Karl  Johans  gt.  47.  N-0162  Oslo  1.  Norway 


Abstract.  The  cellular  localization  of  -y-aminobutyric 
acid  (GABA)  has  been  visualized  immunocytochemically 
in  the  nervous  system  of  Oikopleura  dioica  by  using  an 
antiserum  to  glutaraldehyde  fixation  complexes  of  GABA. 
The  results  show  GABA-like  immunoreactivity  in  neurons 
of  the  brain,  in  cells  of  the  sensory  vesicle,  in  the  caudal 
ganglion,  and  in  the  nerve  cord.  Positive  reactions  were 
also  found  at  the  neuromuscular  terminals  in  the  tail. 

Introduction 

Amino  acids  are  considered  to  be  important  neuro- 
transmitters  in  the  vertebrate  central  nervous  system  (re- 
view: Ottersen  and  Storm-Mathisen,  1984a).  Among 
these,  7-aminobutyric  acid  (GABA)  is  a  dominant  inhib- 
itory neurotransmitter  in  the  brain  and  spinal  cord,  and 
even  occurs  in  the  peripheral  nervous  system  (Jessen  et 
ai.  1986).  GABA  has  also  been  reported  to  be  a  major 
inhibitory  neurotransmitter  in  a  wide  variety  of  the  in- 
vertebrate phyla  (Gerschenfeld,  1973;  Meyer  et  ai.  1986; 
Vitellaro-Zuccarello  and  De  Biasi,  1988).  But  no  infor- 
mation is  available  concerning  this  amino  acid  in  the 
Urochordata,  a  group  that  is  often  considered  to  be  a  phy- 
logenetic  link  between  invertebrates  and  vertebrates. 

The  organization  of  the  nervous  system  of  Oikopleura 
dioica  has  been  investigated  by  several  authors  (Gait  and 
Mackie.  1971;  Holmberg,  1984;  Bollner  et  ai.  1986),  and 
the  presence  of  acetylecholinesterase  in  this  species  has 
been  reported  (Durante,  1959;  Flood,  1973).  The  aim  of 
this  investigation  was  to  establish  whether  Oikopleura 
dioica  exhibits  GABA  immunoreactivity  in  its  central  or 
peripheral  neurons. 

Received  22  January  1990;  accepted  6  November  1990. 

*  Present  address:  Department  of  Biology.  University  of  London.  Royal 
Holloway  and  Bedford  New  College.  Egham  Hill.  Egham,  Surrey  TW20 
OEX,  UK. 


Materials  and  Methods 

Specimens  of  0.  dioica  Fol,  1872.  were  collected  at  the 
Kristineberg  Marine  Biological  Station  and  at  the  Tjarno 
Marine  Biological  Laboratory,  both  on  the  west  coast  of 
Sweden.  For  immunocytochemistry  the  following  fixatives 
were  used:  (1)  5%  glutaraldehyde,  (2)  3%  glutaraldehyde 
and  1%  paraformaldehyde,  or  (3)  1%  glutaraldehyde  and 
1%  paraformaldehyde,  all  in  0.1  A/  sodium-phosphate 
buffer  at  pH  7.4.  After  1  h  fixation  at  room  temperature, 
animals  were  kept  and  transported  in  cold  sodium-phos- 
phate buffer  with  0.5%  glutaraldehyde  added.  Free  floating 
whole  tissues  were  processed  for  immunocytochemistry 
as  described  by  Storm-Mathisen  et  ai  (1983)  and  Dale  et 
ai,  (1986);  the  primary  anti-serum  was  diluted  1:300  be- 
fore processing  according  to  the  peroxidase-anti  peroxi- 
dase  technique  (Sternberger,  1979). 

After  fixation  in  3%  glutaraldehyde  and  1%  paraform- 
aldehyde or  1%  glutaraldehyde  alone,  both  in  0.1  M  so- 
dium-phosphate buffer  at  pH  7.4.  animals  were  embedded 
in  Epon  resin  and  cut  with  a  glass  knife.  One-^m  sections 
were  processed  on  glass  slides  previously  coated  with  either 
chrome  alum  gelatin  or  poly-L-lysine,  and  processed  by 
the  immunogold-silver  (IGS)  method,  as  follows. 

The  sections  were  etched  for  45  min  in  sodium-etha- 
nolate,  washed  3X5  min  in  absolute  alcohol,  followed 
by  2  X  5  min  in  distilled  water,  and  rinsed  briefly  in  20 
mM  Tris  buffer  at  pH  7. 4  containing  155  mMNaCl,  0.1% 
BSA,  and  20  mA/  NaN3 .  The  same  medium  was  also  used 
for  subsequent  rinses  and  for  diluting  sera.  The  sections 
were  then  incubated  with  a  droplet  of  5%  normal  goat 
serum  in  a  moist  chamber  for  20  min.  Thereafter  they 
were  incubated  overnight  in  50  n\  primary  anti-serum 
diluted  1 : 100.  After  a  rinse  in  buffer,  followed  by  washes 
3  X  10  min  in  buffer  at  pH  8.2,  the  sections  were  incubated 
for  60  min  with  GAR  G5  (goat  anti-rabbit  immunoglob- 


119 


120 


T.  BOLLNER  ET  AL 


ulin  adsorbed  to  5  nm  colloidal  gold.  Janssen.  Belgium) 
diluted  1:80  in  the  same  buffer.  Finally  the  sections  were 
washed  with  the  same  medium  as  after  primary  serum, 
rinsed  in  distilled  water  several  times  (5  min  each  at  least), 
developed  in  100  ^1  silver  enhancer  kit  (Janssen),  washed 
in  water,  and  coverslipped. 

Indirect  immunofluorescense  as  well  as  the  peroxidase- 
antiperoxidase  (PAP)  method  of  Sternberger  (1979)  were 
also  tried;  the  same  primary  antibodies  were  visualized 
by  either  a  second  layer  of  FITC-conjugated  sheep  anti- 
rabbit  serum  (SIGMA),  or  by  unlabeled  sheep  anti-rabbit 
serum  (Statens  Bakteriologiska  Laboratorium,  Stockholm) 
followed  by  PAP  complex  (Dakopatts). 

The  antiserum  against  glutaraldehyde-conjugated  7- 
aminobutyric  acid  (GABA  antiserum  26)  was  raised,  pu- 
rified, and  characterized  as  described  previously  (Ottersen 
and  Storm-Mathisen,  1984b;  Ottersen  el  ai.  1986).  For 
all  methods  used,  the  controls  included  absorption  of 
GABA  antiserum  with  GABA-glutaraldehyde  complexes 
(GABA-G)  and  glutamate  complexes  (Glu-G)  at  final 
concentrations  of  300  ^Af.  or  replacement  of  the  primary 
antiserum  with  normal  rabbit  serum.  Furthermore,  the 
immunoreactivity  of  the  antiserum  used  was  tested  ac- 
cording to  the  filter  disc  method  described  by  Ottersen 
and  Storm-Mathisen  (1984b).  The  fixation  conjugates 
spotted  on  the  discs  were  made  from  macromolecules  ex- 
tracted from  rat  brain  homogenate  and  from  homogenate 
of  the  neural  complex  from  the  ascidian  Ciona  intestinalis. 

Results 

The  central  nervous  system  of  Oikopleiira  dioica  con- 
sists of  an  anterior  ganglion  (brain)  and  tail  ganglia.  The 
anterior  part  of  the  brain  is  extended  into  paired  bulbs, 
and  in  the  mid-region  it  has  a  sensory  vesicle.  The  brain 
is  connected  to  several  ganglia  in  the  tail  by  a  solid  nerve 
cord.  The  largest  of  these  tail  ganglia  is  referred  to  as  the 
caudal  ganglion  (Figs.  1,  2). 

Although  the  brain  is  hard  to  see  in  whole  mounts  due 
to  the  thick  oikoplast  epithelium,  GABA-like  immuno- 
reaction  was  observed  in  the  paired  anterior  bulbs  de- 
scribed by  Bollner  el  a/.,  (1986).  In  semithin  sections,  a 
positive  reaction  is  easily  seen  in  some  of  the  neurons  in 
the  bulbs  (Fig.  3).  In  the  rest  of  the  brain,  staining  with 
the  GABA  anti-serum  were  found:  in  one  cell  located 
ventrally  in  the  mid  region,  in  one  of  the  most  caudal 
cells  (Figs.  2,  4),  and  in  a  dorsal  cell  close  to  the  sensory 
vesicle  (Fig.  5).  Furthermore,  immunopositive  staining 
was  seen  in  the  epithelial  cells  referred  to  as  the  "brain 
vesicle  cells"  by  Holmberg  (1984)  (Fig.  6). 

Two  immunostained  cell  bodies  situated  in  the  caudal 
part  of  the  caudal  ganglion  were  observed  in  semithin 
sections  (Fig.  7)  and  in  the  whole  mount  preparations 
(Fig.  8).  Immunoreactive  fibers  were  seen  in  both  semithin 


m 


Figure  1.  Schematic  drawing  of  Oikopleiira  dioica  showing  the  po- 
sition of  the  nervous  system  with  the  immune-positive  neurons  indicated, 
the  position  ot  the  sensory'  vesicle  is  indicated  by  the  dotted  circle,  ab. 
anterior  bulb:  br,  brain;  eg,  caudal  ganglion:  g.  gonads:  i.  intestine:  m, 
mouth:  n.  nerve  cord;  nm.  neuromuscular  junction.  Bar  =  200  Mm. 


sections  (Fig.  7)  and  in  whole  mounts.  Transverse  semithin 
sections  through  the  tail  showed  GABA-like  immuno- 
reactivity in  the  large  nerve  terminals  (Fig.  9)  innervating 
the  muscles  (cf.  Flood,  1973,  1975)  and  in  fibers  of  the 
dorsal  nerve  cord.  Furthermore,  positive  staining  was  of- 
ten seen  in  the  epithelial  cells  of  the  notochord.  Because 
the  tail  of  the  animal  is  twisted  90°  counter-clockwise, 
the  dorsal  nerve  cord  is  seen  to  the  right  of  the  notochord 
in  a  frontal  view  of  a  transverse  section  of  tail. 

The  semithin  sections  from  animals  fixed  with  1%  glu- 
taraldehyde  processed  using  the  immunogold  technique 
gave  a  stronger  reaction  than  any  other  method.  The  PAP- 
method  showed  weak  staining  in  the  anterior  bulbs  and 
in  the  caudal  ganglion,  whereas  the  FITC-incubated  sec- 
tions showed  clear  label  in  the  same  regions  as  did  the 
immunogold  method.  However,  all  methods  showed 
similar  patterns  of  immunoreactivity. 

The  anti  GABA  serum,  either  absorbed  with  glutamate- 
glutaraldehyde  complex  (Glu-G)  or  not,  produced  selec- 
tive staining  of  the  GABA  conjugates  on  the  filter  discs, 
but  no  significant  staining  could  be  seen  after  pretreatment 
of  the  GABA  antiserum  with  GABA-glutaraldehyde 
complex  (GABA-G)  (Fig.  8).  Similar  results  were  obtained 
with  spots  of  amino  acids  conjugated  to  macromolecules 
from  rat  and  Ciona,  suggesting  that  the  previously  dem- 
onstrated specificity  is  valid  also  for  urochordates.  In 
whole  mounts  as  well  as  in  semithin  sections,  the  reaction 
was  virtually  abolished  when  antisera  treated  with  GABA- 
G  or  normal  rabbit  serum  were  used  instead  of  GABA 
antiserum.  Treatment  of  the  anti  serum  with  Glu-G  did 
not  have  this  effect. 

Discussion 

Many  investigations  have  established  the  presence  of 
either  GABA  or  glutamic  acid  decarboxylase  (GAD), 


GABA  IN  OIKOPLKL'RA   NERVOUS  SYSTEM 


121 


•<^ 


IL      br 


Y|Ai-4Q*ij    ' 
,'•'• 


Figure  2.  Sagittal  section  through  the  whole  animal  showing  the  localization  and  GABA-like  immu- 
noreactivity  (arrowheads)  of  the  brain  (br),  one  of  the  anterior  bulbs  (ab).  the  nerve  cord  (n),  the  caudal 
ganglion  (eg)  and  in  a  neuromuscular  junction  of  the  tail  (arrow).  Staining  can  also  be  seen  in  the  gonads 
(g),  at  the  apical  surface  of  some  of  the  intestinal  cells  (i).  and  in  the  rectum  (r).  No  staining  could  be  seen 
in  an  adjacent  section  treated  with  anti-GABA/GABA-G.  1GS  method.  Differential  interference  contrast. 
Bar  =  100  Aim. 


which  catalyzes  the  synthesis  of  GABA  from  glutamate, 
in  lower  chordates  and  in  invertebrates  (Osborne,  1972; 
Osborne  ct  at.,  1979;  De  Biasi,  1986).  More  recently,  the 
use  of  specific  antibodies  (Storm-Mathisen  ct  ai.  1983) 
has  led  to  a  more  precise  knowledge  about  the  cellular 
localization  of  GABA  in  both  vertebrates  (Ottersen  and 
Storm-Mathisen  1984,  a,  b;  Roberts  et  ai.  1987)  and  in- 
vertebrates (Bicker  el  ai.  1985;  Meyer  et  ai.  1986;  Hom- 
berg  et  ai.  1987).  This  is,  to  our  knowledge,  the  first  im- 
munocytochemical  study  on  the  occurrence  of  amino  ac- 
ids in  the  nervous  system  of  a  protochordate.  Using 
biochemical  analyses,  Osborne  et  ai  (1979)  found  GABA 
and  several  other  putative  amino  acid  neurotransmitters 
in  homogenates  of  the  cerebral  ganglion  of  another  pro- 
tochordate, the  tunicate  Ciona  intestinalis. 

The  present  investigation  shows  the  cellular  localization 
of  a  GABA-like  substance  in  the  nervous  tissue  of  O. 
dioica.  The  GABA-positive  cells  in  the  anterior  bulbs  and 
in  the  rest  of  the  brain  are  thought  to  be  neurons  judging 
from  their  location  and  their  ultrastructural  appearance 
previously  described  by  Bollner  et  ai.  (1986,  unpubl.). 
Also,  most  of  the  cells  in  the  caudal  ganglion  are  consid- 
ered to  be  neurons.  However,  one  of  its  anterior  cells,  a 
large  ependymal  cell,  produces  the  Reissner's  fiber 
(Holmberg  and  Olsson,  1984).  The  significance  of  GABA- 
like  immunoreactivity  in  the  neurons  of  the  central  ner- 
vous system  is  difficult  to  evaluate.  Although  they  may 


be  neurons  with  inhibitory  functions,  it  should  be  re- 
membered that  GABA  may  also  have  depolarizing  effects 
(Alger  and  Nicoll,  1982).  In  addition  to  the  neural  local- 
ization, GABA  was  clearly  present  in  epithelial  cells.  This 
agrees  with  the  situation  in  vertebrates,  where  GABA  has 
been  demonstrated  in  non-neural  epithelial  cells  (Oren- 
sanz  el  ai,  1986;  Davanger  et  ai,  1989).  Amino  acids  in 
general  are  also  known  to  modulate  osmoregulation  (see 
Gilles,  1979,  for  review).  Synthesis  of  GABA  has  been 
reported  to  occur  in  fish  erythrocytes  where  it  may  par- 
ticipate in  the  maintenance  of  a  constant  cell  volume  (Fu- 
gelli  et  ai,  1970).  O.  dioica  is  an  isosmotic  animal,  and 
the  vesicle  and  the  chorda  are  the  only  internal  structures 
not  totally  surrounded  by  hemolymph,  and  therefore 
might  use  GABA  for  regulating  the  intracellular  osmo- 
larity. 

The  muscles  in  the  tail  are  innervated  both  by  fibers 
branching  directly  from  the  nerve  cord  and  from  perikarya 
along  the  cord  (Flood,  1973).  These  nerves  have  elaborate 
end-arborizations  on  the  surface  of  the  muscle  cells  and 
are  thought  to  be  cholinergic  (Flood,  1975;  Bone  and 
Mackie.  1982).  Cholinergic  neuromuscular  transmission 
is  widely  distributed  throughout  the  animal  kingdom,  but 
GABA-ergic  inhibition  of  muscles  is  only  known  in  in- 
vertebrate phyla  (Gerschenfeld,  1973).  GABA-like  im- 
munoreactivity has  been  demonstrated  in  inhibitory 
nerves  in  insect  muscle  (Bicker  et  ai,  1988;  Robertson 


122 


T.  BOLLNER  ET  AL 


m 


••• it 


7    nc      k 


a 


\ 


\ 


anti-GABA 


anti-GABA 
+Glu-G 


null 


GABA 


Gly 


Gin 


8 


anti-GABA 
+GABA-G 


Figure  3.  Transverse  section  through  the  anterior  bulbs  with  staining  in  neurons  (arrowheads)  and  in 
some  of  the  fibers  of  the  neuropile  (arrows),  m,  mouth.  IGS  method.  Differential  interference  contrast.  Bar 
=  15  Aim. 

Figure  4.  Sagittal  section  showing  positive  staining  in  one  cell  in  the  mid  part  and  in  one  cell  in  the  rear 
part  of  the  brain  (arrowheads)  and  also  in  a  fiber  of  the  nerve  cord  (arrow).  IGS  method.  Differential 
interference  contrast.  Bar  =  15  ^m. 

Figure  5.  Transverse  section  through  the  brain  in  the  region  of  the  sensory  vesicle  (sv).  with  staining  in 
one  cell  body  (arrowhead).  IGS  method.  Differential  interference  contrast.  Bar  =  15  ^m. 

Figure  6.  Section  through  the  sensory  vesicle  (sv)  showing  positive  staining  in  one  of  the  brain  vesicle 
cells  (arrowhead)  and  in  the  epithelial  cells  forming  the  vesicle  wall.  IGS  method.  Differential  interference 
contrast.  Bar  =  15  ^m. 

Figure  7.  Transverse  section  through  the  caudal  ganglion  showing  positive  staining  in  cell  somata  (ar- 
rowheads) and  in  neuropile  fibers  (arrow);  nc,  notochord.  IGS  method.  Differential  interference  contrast. 
Bar  =  10  jjm. 

Figure  8.  Whole-mount  preparations  of  the  caudal  ganglion  showing  positive  reaction  in  neurons  (arrows) 
after  treatment  with  anti-GABA  (a)  and  anti-GABA/Glu-G  (b)  and  no  reaction  after  treatment  with  anti- 
GABA/GABA-G  (c),  corresponding  control  filter  discs  with  amino  acids  conjugated  to  macromolecules 
from  dona  neural  tissue  by  glutaraldehyde  are  shown  at  the  right.  Gin.  glutamine;  Gly.  glycine;  null, 
glutaraldehyde-treated  protein  with  no  amino  acid  added.  PAP  method.  Bar  =  15  ^m. 


GABA  IN  OlKOPLEUR.-\  NERVOUS  SYSTEM 


123 


Figure  9.  Transverse  section  through  the  tail  near  the  trunk,  frontal  view.  GABA-like  immunoreaction 
can  be  seen  in  a  neuromuscular  terminal  (arrowhead!  and  in  the  nerve  cord  (n).  This  section  also  shows 
staining  in  a  part  of  the  fiber  between  the  nerve  cord  and  the  neuromuscular  terminal  (arrow),  m,  muscle; 
nc,  notochord.  IGS  method.  Differential  interference  contrast.  Bar  =  2(1  j/m 


and  Wisniowski,  1988).  Our  rinding  that  neuromuscular 
synapses  of  O.  dioica  show  GABA-like  immunoreactivity 
is  the  first  indication  that  GABA  may  also  act  as  neuro- 
muscular inhibitory  substance  in  some  chordates. 

More  work  is  needed  to  establish  whether  GABA  is  a 
neurotransmitter  in  O.  dioica.  Its  possible  role  in  osmo- 
regulation  in  the  vesicle  should  also  be  subject  to  further 
investigation. 

Acknowledgments 

We  are  grateful  to  A.  T.  Bore  and  Y.  Lilliemark  for 
technical  assistance.  This  investigation  was  supported  by 
the  Hierta-Retzius  and  Lars  Hiertas  Minne  foundations. 

Literature  Cited 

Alger,  B.  E..  and  R.  A.  Nicoll.  1982.  Pharmacological  evidence  for  two 
kinds  of  GABA  receptor  on  rat  hippocampal  pyramidal  cells  studied 
in  vilro.  J  Physio!  328:  125-141. 

Bicker,  G.,  S.  Schafer,  and  T.  G.  Kingan.  1985.  Mushroom  body  feed- 
back inter  neurons  in  the  honeybee  show  GABA-like  immunoreac- 
tivity. Brain  Res.  360:  394-397. 

Bicker,  G.,  S.  Schafer,  O.  P.  Ottersen,  and  J.  Storm-Mathisen. 
1988.  Glutamate-like  immunoreactivity  in  identified  neuronal 
populations  of  insect  nervous  systems.  J.  Neurosci.  8:  2108-2122. 

Bollner,  T.,  K.  Holmberg,  and  R.  Olsson.  1986.  A  rostral  sensory 
mechanism  in  Oikopleura  dioica  (Appcndiculana).  Acta  Zool  67: 
235-241. 

Bone,  Q.,  and  G.  O.  Mackie,  1982.  Urochordata.  Pp.  473-535  in  Klcc- 
irical  Conduction  and  Behavior  in  "Simple"  Invertebrates.  G.  A.  B. 
Shelton,  ed.  Oxford  University  Press,  Oxford. 

Dale,  N.,  O.  P.  Ottersen,  A.  Roberts,  and  J.  Storm-Mathisen. 
1986.  Inhibitory  neurons  of  a  motor  pattern  generator  in  Xenopus 
revealed  by  antibodies  to  glycine.  Nature  324:  255-257. 

Davanger,  S.,  O.  P.  Ottersen,  and  J.  Storm-Mathisen.  1989.  GABA 
immunoreactive  cells  in  the  rat  gastrointestinal  epithelium.  Anal. 
Embryo!  179:  221-226. 


I)e  Biasi,  S.,  C.  Frassoni,  and  I,.  Vitellaro  Zuccarello.  1986.  Glutamic 
acid  decarboxylase  (GAD)-like  immunoreactivity  in  the  pedal  gan- 
glion of  Myiiliis  galloprovincialis.  Cell  Tissue  Res  244:  591-593. 

Durantc,  M.  1959.  Sulla  localizzazione  istochimica  della  acetilcolines- 
terasi  lungo  lo  sviluppo  di  alcune  ascidie  e  in  appendicularie.  Acta 
Lmbryol.  Morphol  Exp  2:  234-243. 

Flood,  P.  R.  1973.  Ultrastructural  and  cytochemical  studies  on  the 
muscle  innervation  in  Appendiculana.  Tunicata.  J  Microsc.  18:  317- 
326. 

Flood,  P.  R.  1975.  Scanning  electron  microscope  observations  on  the 
muscle  innervation  of  Oikopleura  dioica  Fol  (Appendiculana,  Tun- 
icata) with  notes  on  the  arrangement  of  connective  tissue  fibers.  Cell 
Tissue  Res  164:  357-369. 

Fol,  II.  1872.  Etudes  sur  les  appendiculaires  du  detroit  de  Messine. 
Mem.  Soc.  Phys.  Hist.  Nat.  Geneve  21:  445-449. 

Fugelli,  K.,  J.  Storm-Mathisen,  and  F.  Fonnum.  1970.  Synthesis  of 
vaminobutyric  acid  in  fish  erythrocytes.  Nature  228:  1001. 

Gait,  C.  P.,  and  G.  O.  Mackie.  1971.  Electrical  correlates  of  ciliary 
reversal  in  Oikopleura.  J.  Exp.  Biol.  55:  205-212. 

Gerschenfeld,  II.  M.  1973.  Chemical  transmission  in  invertebrate  cen- 
tral nervous  system  and  neuromuscular  junctions.  Physiol.  Rev  53: 
1-1  19. 

Gilles,  R.  1979.  Intracellular  organic  osmotic  effectors.  Pp.  1 1-154  in 
Mechanisms  ofOsmoregulation  in  Animals.  R.  Gilles,  ed.  Wiley  Chi- 
chester.  New  York. 

Holmberg,  K.  1984.  A  transmission  electron  microscopic  investigation 
of  the  sensory  vesicle  in  the  brain  of  Oikopleura  dioica  (Appendi- 
cularia).  Zoomorpliology  104:  298-303. 

Holmberg,  K.,  and  R.  Olsson.  1984.  The  origin  of  Reissner's  fibre  in 
the  appendicularian.  Oikopleura  dioica.  1'iddensk.  Aledd.  Dan.  Na- 
lurlusl.  Foren.  145:  43-52. 

Homberg,  U.,  T.  G.  Kingan,  and  J.  G.  Ilildebrand. 
1987.  Immunocytochemistry  of  GABA  in  the  brain  and  sub- 
oesophageal  ganglion  of  Manduea  sexta.  Cell  Tissue  Res.  248:  1-24. 

Jessen,  K.  R.,  J.  M.  Hills,  and  J.  Saffrey.  1986.  Immunohistochemical 
demonstration  of  GABA  ergeic  neurons  in  the  enteric  nervous  system. 
J.  Neurosci  6:  1628-1634. 

Meyer,  E.  P.,  C.  Matute,  P.  Streit,  and  D.  R.  Nassel.  1986.  Insect 
optic  lobe  neurons  identifiable  with  monoclonal  antibodies  to  GABA. 
Htslochemistrv  84:  207-2 16. 


124 


T.  BOLLNER  KT  AL 


Orensanz,  L.  M.,  I.  Fernandez,  R.  Martin  del  Rio,  and  J.  Storm-Malhisen. 
1986.  Gamma-aminohutync  acid  in  the  rat  oviduct.  Adv.  Biocliem. 
Psychophamacol.  42:  265-274. 

Osborne,  N.  N.  1972.  Occurrence  of  glycine  and  glutamic  acid  in  the 
nervous  system  of  two  fish  species  and  some  invertebrates.  Coinp. 
Biocliem  I'hyvol  43B:  579-585. 

Osborne,  N.  N.,  V.  Neuhoff,  E.  Ewers,  and  H.  A.  Robertson. 
1979.  Putative  neurotransmitters  in  the  cerebral  ganglia  of  the  tu- 
nicate Ciiina  intestinalis.  Coinp.  Biocliem.  Physiol.  63C:  209-213. 

Ottersen,  O.  P.,  and  J.  Storm-Mathisen.  1984a.  Neurons  containing 
or  accumulating  transmitter  amino  acids.  Pp.  141-146  in  Classical 
Transmitters  ami  Transmitter  Receptors  in  the  CMS  Purl  II.  A. 
Bjorklund,  T.  Hokfelt,  and  M.  J.  Kuhar,  eds.  Handbook  of  Chemical 
Neuroanatomy.  Vol.  3  Elsevier,  Amsterdam. 

Ottersen,  O.  P.,  and  J.  Storm-Mathisen.  1984b.  Glutamate-  and 
GABA-contaming  neurons  in  the  mouse  and  rat  brain,  as  demon- 
strated with  a  new  immunocytochemical  technique.  J.  Coinp.  Neural. 
229:  374-392. 


Ottersen,  O.  P.,  J.  Storm-Mathisen,  S.  Madsen,  S.  Skumlien,  and  J. 
Stremhaug.  1986.  Evaluation  of  the  immunocytochemical  method 
for  amino  acids.  Med.  Biol.  64:  147-158. 

Roberts,  A.,  N.  Dale,  O.  P.  Ottersen,  and  J.  Storm-Mathisen.  1987.  The 
early  development  of  neurons  with  GABA  immunoreactivity  in  CNS 
of  X'enopus  laevis  embryos.  J  Coinp.  N  enrol.  261:  435-449. 

Robertson,  R.  M.,  and  I.,  \\isniowski.  1988.  GABA-like  immuno- 
reactivity of  identified  inter  neurons  in  the  flight  system  of  the  locust. 
Locusta  migraloria.  Cell  Tissue  Res  254:  331-340. 

Sternberger,  I,.  A.  1979.  Immune/cytochemistry,  Second  Edition.  John 
Wiley  and  Sons,  New  York.  Chichester. 

Storm-Mathisen,  J.,  A.  K.  Leknes,  A.  T.  Bore,  J.  L.  Vaaland,  P.  Ed- 
minson.  F-M.  S.  Haug,  and  O.  P.  Ottersen.  1983.  First  visualization 
of  glutamate  and  GABA  in  neurons  by  immunocytochemistry.  Nature 
301:  517-520. 

Vitcllaro  Zuccarello,  I,.,  and  S.  De  Biasi.  1988.  GABA-like  immu- 
noreactivity in  the  pedal  ganglia  of  Mytilus  galloprovmciulis-  light 
and  electron  microscopic  study.  J.  Coinp.  Neural.  267:  516-524. 


Reference:  Biol  Bull  180:  125-134.  (February, 


Ontogeny  of  Osmoregulation  and  Salinity  Tolerance  in 

Cancer  irroratus;  Elements  of  Comparison  with 

C.  borealis  (Crustacea,  Decapoda) 

G.  CHARMANTIER  AND  M.  CHARMANTIER-DAURES 

Laboratoire  d'Ecop/iysiologie  des  Invertebres,  Universite  des  Sciences.  Montpellier  2. 
Place  E.  Bataillon.  3409?  Montpellier  tedex  05.  France 


Abstract.  Osmoregulation  and  salinity  tolerance  were 
studied  in  zoeae,  megalopae,  first  crab  stage  (osmoregu- 
lation  only),  and  adults  of  Cancer  irroratus,  and  in  zoeae 
and  adults  of  C.  borealis. 

In  C.  irroratus.  salinity  tolerance  was  moderate  in  zoeae, 
decreased  in  late  zoeae  5,  was  at  a  minimum  in  megalopae. 
and  increased  in  adults.  The  lower  and  upper  lethal  sa- 
linities for  50%  of  the  animals  (48  h  LS  50)  at  15°C  were 
about  13-17%o/42-50%o  in  zoeae,  24%o/37%o  in  mega- 
lopae, and  8.5%o/65%o  in  adults. 

In  C.  borealis.  the  corresponding  values  of  LS  50s  were 
16-20%o/46-50%o  in  zoeae  and  12%«/65%o  in  adults. 

In  both  species,  zoeae  were  hyper-osmoconformers; 
adults  were  isosmotic  in  high  salinities  and  slightly  hyper- 
regulators  in  low  salinities.  In  C.  irroratus.  the  change 
from  larval  to  adult  type  of  regulation  occurred  from 
megalopa  (hyper-osmoconformer)  to  first  crab  stage  (hy- 
per-regulator in  dilute  media),  i.e..  after  the  completion 
of  metamorphosis. 

Osmoregulation  and  salinity  tolerance  appear  corre- 
lated and  are  modified  at  metamorphosis.  These  results 
are  discussed  with  an  emphasis  on  the  effects  of  meta- 
morphosis on  Osmoregulation  of  developing  decapods. 

Introduction 

While  Osmoregulation  has  been  extensively  studied  in 
adult  crustaceans  (review  in  Mantel  and  Farmer,  1983), 
relatively  few  data  are  available  on  larval  or  post-larval 
Osmoregulation,  in  the  following  species:  Rhilhropanopeus 
harrisii  (Kalber  and  Costlow,  1966),  Cardisoma  guan- 
humi  (Kalber  and  Costlow,  1968),  Callinectes  sapidus. 

Received  1 1  June  1990;  accepted  27  November  1990. 


Hepalus  ep/ie/iticiis.  Lihinia  cinarginata  (Kalber,  1970), 
Sesaniui  reticii/atnm  (Foskett,  1977),  Clibanarim  vittatus 
(Young,  1979),  Callianassa  Jamaica  (Felderc/  a/.,  1986), 
Alacrohrac/iiuni  pelersi  (Read,  1984),  Uca  snhcylindrica 
(Rabalais  and  Cameron,  1985),  Homarus  americanus,  and 
Penaeus  japonicm  (Charmantier,  1986;  Charmantier  et 
a/..  1984a,  1988). 

For  different  reasons,  including  culture  difficulties  in 
late  larval  stages,  Osmoregulation  was  frequently  studied 
in  larval  stages  only,  particularly  in  brachyuran  crabs. 
However,  in  the  four  latter  species,  Osmoregulation  was 
studied  throughout  the  post-embryonic  development  in- 
cluding post-metamorphic  stages,  thus  Osmoregulation  in 
larvae,  postlarvae,  and  adults  could  be  compared.  The 
ability  to  osmoregulate  did  not  change  along  the  devel- 
opment in  some  species.  In  other  species,  metamorphosis 
marked  the  appearance  of  the  adult  type  of  regulation; 
among  brachyurans,  this  case  has  been  so  far  documented 
in  only  one  species,  U.  snhcylindrica,  the  adults  of  which 
are  strong  hyper-hypo-regulators  (Rabalais  and  Cameron, 
1985). 

Consequently,  one  of  the  objectives  of  this  study  was 
to  determine  whether  comparable  changes  in  Osmoregu- 
lation at  metamorphosis  exist  in  brachyurans  with  lower 
abilities  to  osmoregulate,  particularly  in  species  of  the  ge- 
nus Cancer,  which  slightly  hyper-regulate  in  low  salinities. 
Experiments  designed  to  study  the  ontogeny  of  Osmoreg- 
ulation were  performed  on  the  most  abundant  Cancer 
species  of  the  Canadian  East  coast,  the  rock  crab  Cancer 
irroratus  Say  1817.  Comparative  data  were  also  obtained 
from  some  developmental  stages  of  a  sympatric  species, 
the  Jonah  crab  Cancer  borealis  Stimpson,  1859. 

Numerous  studies  have  dealt  with  the  tolerance  to  sa- 
linity of  crustacean  larvae  and  postlarvae,  but  the  corre- 


125 


126 


G.  CHARMANTIER  AND  M.  CHARMANTIER-DAURES 


lation  between  the  salinity  tolerance  of  different  devel- 
opmental stages  and  their  corresponding  osmoregulatory 
capabilities  has  only  been  experimentally  investigated  in 
a  few  species,  U.  subcylindrica  (Rabalais  and  Cameron. 
1985),  H.  americann.s  and  P.  japonicus  (Charmantier  et 
al.,  1988). 

Thus,  the  second  objective  of  this  study  was  to  deter- 
mine the  salinity  tolerance  of  larval,  postlarval,  and  adult 
stages  of  C.  irroratus  (and,  for  the  purpose  of  comparison, 
of  C.  borealis).  and  to  attempt  to  correlate  their  osmo- 
regulatory abilities  and  their  salinity  tolerance.  This  is  of 
particular  interest  in  Cancer  species,  the  larvae  of  which 
are  submitted  to  different  patterns  of  salinity  changes:  in 
C.  irroratus,  zoeae  hatch  offshore  and,  as  development 
proceeds,  late  larvae  and  postlarvae  are  found  nearer  to 
shore  (Sandifer,  1973). 

In  both  species,  the  early  post-embryonic  development 
comprises  one  prezoea,  five  zoeal  stages,  one  megalopal 
stage,  and  several  early  crab  stages  (Sastry,  1977a,  b).  Fol- 
lowing a  wide  acceptance,  zoeae  are  larval  stages,  and  the 
following  stages,  beginning  with  the  megalopa,  are  con- 
sidered postlarvae  (Felder  et  al.,  1985). 

In  C.  irroratus,  osmoregulation  has  been  studied  in 
adults  and  large  juveniles  (Thurbergrf  al.,  1973;  Cantelmo 
etai.  1975;Neufeldand  Pritchard,  1979),  and  preliminary 
data  on  osmoregulation  are  also  available  in  the  early  post- 
embryonic  stages  (Charmantier  et  al.,  1989). 


Materials  and  Methods 


Animals 


Adult  C.  irroratus  and  C.  borealis  were  caught  by 
SCUBA  diving  in  summer  and  autumn  in  Passama- 
quoddy  Bay  and  transferred  to  the  culture  facility  at  the 
Biological  Station,  St  Andrews,  New  Brunswick,  Canada. 
They  were  kept  in  tanks  supplied  with  running  seawater 
(S  ^  29-32%o;  T  s  12°C)  under  natural  photoperiod,  and 
were  fed  cod,  squid,  and  shrimp  (Pandalus  borealis).  In 
September,  two  weeks  before  the  experiments,  four  groups 
of  crabs  were  selected:  large  and  small  C.  irroratus  [ceph- 
alothoracic  width  (CTW):  91  to  115  mm  and  45  to  70 
mm,  respectively],  and  large  and  small  C.  borealis  (CTW: 
80-110  mm  and  57-71  mm).  Males  and  females  were 
equally  represented  in  each  group.  They  were  transferred 
to  250-1  tanks  with  charcoal-nitrated  recirculated  seawater 
kept  at  15°C.  Only  animals  in  molt  stages  C  or  Do  (ac- 
cording to  the  nomenclature  of  Drach,  1939)  were  retained 
for  survival  and  osmoregulatory  experiments. 

Larvae  of  both  species  were  obtained  in  spring  and 
summer  from  some  of  the  aforementioned  crabs.  After 
hatching,  larvae  were  transferred  to  40-1  planktonkreisels 
(Hughes  et  al.,  1974)  supplied  with  flow-through  seawater 
at  a  salinity  of  29-32%o  under  natural  photoperiod.  The 
planktonkreisels,  normally  used  for  culturing  lobster  lar- 


vae, were  modified  for  the  culture  of  crab  larvae.  Seawater 
was  filtered  to  50  ^m  during  the  zoea  development  then 
suppressed;  flow-rate  was  set  at  2.5-3  1  min"'  from  zoeae 
1  to  early  megalops,  then  at  1.-1.5  1  min"1;  a  280  ^m 
mesh  screen  was  used  around  the  overflow  system.  Water 
temperature  was  set  at  15°C  during  the  zoeal  develop- 
ment, then  at  19°C.  Cephalothoracic  length  was  about 
0.56  mm  in  zoeae  1,  1.5  mm  in  zoeae  5,  2.2  mm  in  mega- 
lopae,  and  CTW  was  2.3  mm  in  first  crab  stage  (Char- 
mantier-Daures  and  Charmantier,  199 1 ).  Crab  larvae  were 
fed  three  times  a  day  with  live  Anemia  nauplii.  Larvae 
of  C.  irroratus  were  cultured  to  the  second  crab  stage,  and 
those  of  C.  borealis  to  the  third  zoea.  As  each  larval  stage 
lasts  several  days,  molting  stages  were  obtained  according 
to  the  time  elapsed  from  the  preceeding  molt,  and  three 
groups  of  animals,  postmolt  stage  A.  stage  C,  and  premolt 
stage  D.  were  selected. 

Preparatit  >n  <  if  media 

Experimental  media  were  prepared  in  compartmented 
250-1  tanks  for  adults  and  0.5-1  plastic  containers  for  larvae 
and  young  crabs.  Dilute  media  were  prepared  by  adding 
tap  water  to  seawater,  and  high  salinity  media  were  pre- 
pared by  adding  "Instant  Ocean  Synthetic  Sea  Salts" 
(Aquarium  Systems,  Inc.)  to  seawater.  All  experiments 
were  conducted  at  15°C.  Salinities  were  expressed  ac- 
cording to  the  osmotic  pressure  in  mosm-kg  ',  and  to 
the  salt  content  in  the  medium  in  %o.  A  value  of  3.4%o  is 
equivalent  to  100  mosm-kg"1.  Osmotic  pressure  was 
measured  with  an  Advanced  Instruments  3 1  LA  or  Wes- 
cor  5000  osmometer,  and  salinity  on  a  YSI  33  salinometer. 

Survival  bioassays 

Due  to  the  small  number  of  available  animals,  salinity 
tolerance  in  adults  was  evaluated  only  from  the  number 
of  surviving  and  dead  animals  in  media  of  different  salin- 
ities. Adult  crabs  were  progressively  adapted  from  seawater 
to  diluted  or  concentrated  media  by  adding  freshwater  or 
Instant  Ocean  salts  to  the  original  medium;  each  change 
of  100  mosm-kg"1  in  the  salinity  required  about  24  h. 
Between  two  changes  of  salinity,  they  were  kept  for  two 
days  at  constant  salinity  in  each  test  medium,  which  dif- 
fered from  one  another  by  increments  of  1 00  mosm  •  kg" ' 
(=3.4%o). 

Acute  static  48-96  h  bioassays  were  conducted  with 
zoeae  and  megalopae  held  in  test  media  ranging  from  100 
mosm  •  kg" '  to  seawater  ( ^900- 1 000  mosm  •  kg" ' )  and  to 
1600  mosm-kg"1,  and  differing  by  increments  of  100 
mosm -kg  '.  Each  bioassay  was  run  on  a  group  of  10 
individuals  and  replicated.  Animals  were  counted  and 
dead  animals  removed  at  0.5,  1,  3,  6,  12,  24,  36,  48,  72, 
96  h  according  to  the  prescriptions  of  Sprague  (1969)  in 
toxicity  studies.  The  criteria  for  death  were  total  lack  of 


ONTOGENY  OF  CANCER  OSMOREGULAT1ON 


127 


600 
20 


1000 
30  40 


1800      mosm. kg 
60  n/« 


Medium 

Figure  1.  Salinity  tolerance  in  adult  Cancer  irn»atu,\  and  C  horciilis  at  15°C.  Percent  mortality  of 
animals  according  to  the  salinity  of  the  medium.  Number  of  animals  at  the  start  of  the  experiments  in 
seawater  (SW):  C'  irnmiltis:  16  (to  low  salinity  media)  and  8  (to  high  salinity  media);  C  borealis:  19  and  7. 


movement,  immobility  of  appendages  and  heart,  and  lack 
of  response  after  repeated  touches  with  a  probe.  Median 
lethal  salinities  (LS  50)  and  95%  confidence  intervals  were 
calculated  by  techniques  of  probit  analysis  (Lichtfield  and 
Wilcoxon,  1949;  Finney,  1962)  computerized  on  the  Let- 
cur  program  (Zitko,  1982;  Lieberman,  1983).  LS  50s  were 
calculated  at  24,  48,  and  96  h.  Survival  bioassays  were 
not  run  in  first  and  second  crab  stages  due  to  the  small 
number  of  available  animals. 

Osmoregulation 

The  hemolymph  was  collected  from  adult  crabs  via  a 
hypodermic  needle  inserted  through  the  articulation 
membrane  at  the  basis  of  the  fourth  or  fifth  pereiopods. 
At  least  seven  days  elapsed  between  hemolymph  samples 
were  taken  from  the  same  animal. 

Zoeae,  megalopae,  and  young  crabs  were  quickly  dried 
on  filter  paper  and  immersed  in  mineral  oil  to  avoid  evap- 
oration and  desiccation.  The  hemolymph  was  then  sam- 
pled with  a  glass  micropipette  inserted  in  the  heart. 

Osmotic  pressure  of  hemolymph  was  measured  on  an 
Advanced  Instruments  31  LA  or  Wescor  5000  osmometer 
(adults)  or  on  a  Kalber-Clifton  micro-osmometer,  with 
reference  to  the  osmotic  pressure  of  the  medium  (young 
stages).  Student  /  tests  were  used  for  statistical  compari- 
sons. 


Results 


Salinity  tolerance 


The  ability  of  C.  irroratus  and  C.  borealis  to  tolerate 
low  and  high  salinities  varied  with  post-embryonic  de- 
velopment. 

Adults  of  C.  irroratus  survived  without  mortality  in 
media  ranging  from  500  mosm  •  kg" '  to  1 300  mosm  •  kg" ' 


(=17%»  to  44%o).  The  LS  50  s  were  about  250  and  1900 
mosm -kg"1  (s8.5  and  65%o).  In  adult  C.  borealis.  no 
mortality  was  observed  between  600  and  1300 
mosm -kg"1  (^20.4  and  44%o),  and  LS  50s  were  about 
350  and  1900  mosm -kg"'  (12  and  65%o)  (Fig.  1).  No 
difference  in  salinity  tolerance  was  detected  between  large 
and  small  crabs  of  either  species. 

In  larvae  and  postlarvae  of  C.  irroratus  the  48  h  LS  50 
in  low  salinity  media  varied  around  450  ±  60  mosm  •  kg~' 
(^15  ±  2%o)  in  zoeal  stages  1  to  4  and  early  5,  then  in- 
creased from  the  end  of  stage  zoea  5  through  early  mega- 
lopae (^600  mosm -kg"1,  20%o)  to  a  highly  significant 
maximum  value  (corresponding  to  a  minimum  tolerance) 
of  700  mosm  •  kg"1  (=24%o)  in  intermolt  megalopae.  The 
24  h  and  96  h  LS  50s  were,  respectively,  generally  lower 
and  higher  than  the  48  h  value  but  followed  the  same 
pattern  of  variation.  Maximum  LS  50s  at  24,  48,  and  96 
h  occurred  in  megalopae  with  respective  values  of  520, 
700,  and  820  mosm -kg"1  (18,  24,  and  28%o).  differing 
significantly  from  one  another. 

In  high  salinity  media,  the  48  h  LS  50  of  C.  irroratus 
young  stages  varied  around  1350±  120  mosm -kg"' (^46 
±  4%o)  in  zoeal  stages  1  to  5,  then  decreased  in  early  meg- 
alopae (^1240  mosm  •  kg"1,  ^42%o)  to  a  highly  significant 
minimum  value  of  1 100  mosm  •  kg"1  (^37%o)  in  intermolt 
megalopae.  The  24  h  and  96  h  LS  50s  were  respectively 
higher  and  lower  than  the  48  h  value;  they  followed  the 
same  pattern  of  variation,  decreasing  to  minima  of  1 150 
mosm -kg"1  (24  h:  39%o)  and  1000  mosm -kg"1  (96  h: 
34%o)  in  megalopae  (Fig.  2). 

In  zoeae  of  C.  borealis.  the  48  h  LS  50  varied  around 
530  ±  60  mosm -kg"1  (^18  ±  2%»)  in  low  salinities.  The 
24  h  and  96  h  LS  50s  were  markedly  lower  and  higher 
than  the  48  h  LS  50.  The  differences  between  96  h  and 
24  h  LS  50s  were  more  important  than  in  C.  irroratus:  in 


128 


G.  CHARMANTIER  AND  M.  CHARMANTIER-DAURES 


700     6 


A       C 
1 Zl  - 


DA     C      DA     C      DA     C      DA 
— ' — Z2— ' — Z3— •— Z4- 


C 
-Z5- 


DA 


C 

-Mgl  — 


300 

Molt  stages 
L.  stages 
Days 


Figure  2.  Salinity  tolerance  in  zoeae  1-5  and  megalopae  of  Cancer  irrorulti.i  at  15°C.  Variations  in  LS 
50  in  %o  and  mosm  •  kg"'  according  to  larval  and  molt  stages  and  to  days  of  development,  in  high  and  low 
salinities  (upper  and  lower  traces).  Each  point  represents  the  mean  value  of  at  least  two  determinations  from 
10  animals,  with  95%  confidence  interval.  Closed  triangles:  24  h  LS  50.  Closed  circles:  48  h  LS  50;  open 
circles:  96  h  LS  50. 


some  instances  (early  zoea  1  and  zoea  2),  the  24  LS  50 
was  about  300  mosm  •  kg"'  ( 10%o),  the  96  h  LS  50  reaching 
about  740  mosm  •  kg  '  (25%).  In  high  salinities,  the  48  h 
LS  50  varied  around  1420  ±  60  mosm  •  kg" '  (^48  ±  2%o); 
24  h  and  96  h  LS  50s  were  respectively  higher  and  lower 
(Fig.  3). 

Osmoregulation 

Adaptation  time.  The  time  of  adaptation  after  a 
change  in  the  environmental  salinity  was  evaluated  in 
stage  C  zoeae  1  and  5  and  in  adults  of  C.  irroratus.  After 
a  rapid  transfer  from  seawater  at  920  mosm  •  kg"1  (31%o) 


to  a  dilute  medium  of  500  mosm -kg  '  (17%o),  the  he- 
molymph  osmotic  pressure  stabilized  within  1  to  2  h  in 
zoeae  1  and  5.  In  adults  transferred  from  seawater  to  a 
dilute  medium  of  677  mosm -kg"'  (23%o),  the  corre- 
sponding time  was  24  h  (Fig.  4).  The  time  of  osmotic 
adaptation  to  concentrated  media  was  not  tested;  in  other 
species,  it  was  shorter  than  the  time  of  adaptation  to  dilute 
media  (Charmantier  ct  ai.  1988).  In  all  subsequent  ex- 
periments and  in  both  species,  we  kept  the  young  stages 
6-24  h  and  the  adults  3-4  days  in  each  medium  before 
sampling. 

Osmoregulation.     Adults  of  C.  irroratus  and  C.  bo- 
realis  were  almost  osmoconformers  in  high  salinities  and 


ONTOGENY  OF  CANCKR  OSMOREGULATION 


129 


700     ~ 
O 

6 


-zi 


DA 


C 
-22- 


C  Molt  stages 

L  stages 


20 


25        Days 


Figure3.  Salinity  tolerance  in  zoeae  [-3  of  Cancer  borealis  at  15°C. 
Variations  in  LS  50  in  %»  and  mosm  •  kg"'  according  to  larval  and  molt 
stages  and  to  days  of  development,  in  high  and  low  salinities  (upper  and 
lower  traces).  Each  point  represents  the  mean  value  of  two  determinations 
from  10  animals,  with  95%  confidence  interval.  Closed  triangles:  24  h 
LS  50;  closed  circles:  48  h  LS  50;  open  circles:  96  h  LS  50. 


seawater,  and  their  regulation  was  slightly  hyper-osmotic 
in  dilute  media  (Fig.  5).  No  difference  of  hyper-regulation 
was  detected  between  large  and  small  adults  in  C.  bomilis. 
In  C.  irroratus  large  crabs  were  significantly  stronger  reg- 
ulators than  small  crabs  (hemolymph-medium  differences 
of  7 1  ±  7  and  55  ±  1 3  mosm  •  kg"1  respectively,  P  <  0.005, 
in  a  500  mosm  •  kg"1;  17%o,  medium).  The  ability  to  hy- 
per-regulate in  dilute  media  was  significantly  higher  in  C. 
irroratus  than  in  C.  borealis  (in  large  crabs,  hemolymph- 
medium  differences  of  71  ±7  and  37  ±  5  mosm-kg  ',  P 
<  0.001,  in  a  500  mosm -kg"1,  17%o,  medium). 

Zoeae  1  to  5  of  C.  irroratus  in  molting  stage  C  hyper- 
osmoconformed  at  almost  all  tested  salinities,  i.e.,  their 
hemolymph  osmotic  pressure  varied  as  a  function  of  ex- 
ternal osmotic  pressure  but  remained  above  external  by 
about  1 5-50  mosm  •  kg"1;  at  the  lowest  tested  salinity  (300 
mosm  •  kg"1),  zoeae  were  isosmotic.  In  some  zoeal  stages. 


regulation  in  dilute  media  was  slightly  more  hyper-osmotic 
in  premolt  ( zoeae  1 ,4  at  500  mosm  •  kg" ' :  P  <  0.05 )  and 
less  hyper-osmotic  in  post-molt  (zoeae  2,  3,  4  at  500  and 
900  mosm  •  kg" ':  P  <  0.0 1 ).  Megalopae  were  also  hyper- 
osmoconformers.  The  pattern  of  osmoregulation  seemed 
to  change  after  the  completion  of  metamorphosis.  First 
crab  stages  were  almost  osmoconformers  in  seawater  and 
their  regulation  was  slightly  hyper-osmotic  in  a  dilute  me- 
dium of  500  mosm  •  kg  '.  1 1%«  (hemolymph-medium  dif- 
ference of  55  ±  16  mosm-kg"')  (Fig.  6). 

Zoeae  1  to  3  of  C.  borealis  had  the  same  pattern  of 
hyperosmocon  form  ing  regulation  as  zoeae  of  C.  irroratus 
(Fig.  7). 


Discussion 


Sa/initv  tolerance 


In  Cancer  uroraliis,  the  interval  of  tolerable  salinities 
tends  to  decrease  at  the  end  of  the  larval  development 
and  is  minimum  in  megalopae.  At  this  stage,  the  96  h  LS 
50s  are  about  28%o  and  34%o,  which  means  that  megalopae 
are  almost  restricted  to  seawater.  In  adults,  approximate 
48  h  LS  50s  are  8.5%o  and  65%o.  The  wide  euryhalinity 
demonstrated  by  adults  could  be  partly  related  to  the  rel- 
atively short  time  of  exposure  to  the  different  media  and 
to  the  progressive  adaptation  to  changing  salinities.  Long- 
term  exposure  to  extreme  salinities  could  yield  more  re- 
strictive results. 

These  results  are  in  agreement  with  previous  data.  Sas- 
try  (1970)  found  that  at  15°C,  only  5.5%  of  megalopae 
of  C.  irroratus  molted  to  the  first  crab  stage  in  a  medium 
of  15%o,  while  this  molt  was  successful  in  a  higher  per- 
centage of  megalopae  in  media  ranging  from  20  to  35%o, 
with  a  maximum  rate  of  76%  in  a  medium  of  30%o.  Com- 
plete development  from  zoea  1  to  first  crab  stage  was 
found  possible  at  15°C  between  20  and  35%o  (Sastry  and 
McCarthy,  1973)  or  25  and  35%o,  but  survival  exceeded 
50%  only  in  30-35%.  (Johns,  1981).  In  adults,  McCluskey 
(1975,  cited  in  Bigford,  1979)  found  survival  was  possible 
for  three  days  at  5-8°C  in  salinities  ranging  from  10  to 
20%o  (the  upper  limit  was  not  tested). 

Compared  to  the  larvae  of  C.  irroratus,  zoeae  of  C. 
borealis  were  less  tolerant  to  prolonged  exposure  to  low 
salinities.  In  C.  borealis,  Sastry  and  McCarthy  (1973) 
found  that  complete  larval  development  was  only  possible 
in  a  medium  of  30%»  at  20°C.  These  and  our  results  dem- 
onstrate that  C.  borealis  is  more  stenohaline  than  C  ir- 
roratus, and,  in  particular,  less  tolerant  to  low  salinities 
during  the  larval  development  and  in  adults. 

Adaptation  time 

In  C.  irroratus.  the  time  of  osmotic  equilibration  in  a 
dilute  medium  is  about  1  to  2  h  in  larvae  and  24  h  in 


130 


G.  CHARMANTIER   AND  M.  CHARMANTIER-DAURES 


Q. 

E 


Figure  4.  Change  in  hemolymph  osmotic  pressure  in  stages  zoeae  1  and  5  of  Cancer  irroratus  after  rapid 
transfer  from  seawater  (920  mosm  •  kg"',  31%»)  to  a  dilute  medium  (500  mosm  •  kg"',  17%o),  and  in  adults 
of  C.  irroratus  after  rapid  transfer  from  seawater  to  677  mosm  •  kg~',  23%o.  at  1 5°C.  Each  point  represents 
the  mean  value  of  determinations  from  3  to  5  zoeae  or  5  adults,  with  95%  confidence  interval. 


adults.  Adaptation  time  is  thus  size-dependent,  which 
could  be  related  to  differences  in  the  volumes  of  water 
and  ion  exchanges  and  to  differences  in  tegument  per- 
meability between  development  stages.  These  times  of 
adaptation  are  similar  to  those  of  the  corresponding  stages 
of  other  species  (see  Charmantier  el  ai,  1988). 

Osmoregulation 

In  C.  irroratus  and  C.  borealis,  adults  osmoconform  in 
high  salinities  and  seawater  and  slightly  hyper-regulate  in 
dilute  media.  The  ability  to  hyper-regulate  is  higher  in  C. 
irroratus. 

A  similar  pattern  of  osmoregulation  has  been  described 
in  C.  irroratus  and  other  species  of  Cancer,  but  the  ability 
to  hyper-regulate  varies  with  the  species,  although  other 
factors  such  as  size  and  temperature  can  affect  this  pa- 
rameter. In  media  of  approximately  500  mosm -kg"', 
17%o,  at  temperatures  of  =15-20°C,  the  difference  be- 
tween the  osmotic  pressures  of  hemolymph  and  medium 
expressed  in  mosm -kg"'  is  about  15  in  C.  antennarius 
(Jones,  1941),  30-40  in  C  borealis  (this  study),  50  in  C. 
pagurus  (Wanson  et  ai,  1983),  50-120  in  C.  irroratus 
(Thurberg  et  al..  1973;  Cantelmo  et  ai,  1975;  Neufeld 
and  Pritchard,  1979;  this  study),  150-250  in  C.  nwgister 


(Jones,  1941;  Engelhardt  and  Dehnel,  1973;  Hunter  and 
Rudy.  1975). 

Zoeae  of  C.  irroratus  hyper-osmoconform  in  all  tested 
salinities.  Most  decapod  larvae  that  have  been  studied 


Medium   (mosm. kg      ) 

Figure  5.  Variations  in  the  difference  between  the  osmotic  pressures 
of  hemolymph  and  medium  according  to  the  osmotic  pressure  of  the 
medium  in  large  and  small  adults  of  Cancer  irroralus  and  C.  borealis  at 
I5°C.  Each  point  represents  the  mean  value  of  determinations  from  7 
to  10  animals  (exception  in  lowest  salinity:  4-6  animals)  with  95%  con- 
fidence interval. 


ONTOGENY  OF  CANCER  OSMOREGULATION 


131 


80 


40 


20 


E      60 


T) 
« 

I 


20 


20 


0   I 


Zl 


Z2 


300          500         700         900         1100        1300 

Medium     (moim.kg-1) 


60 


20 


^      40 
E 
1       20 


40 

-c 
a. 

I     20 


40  . 


20 


o  L 


Cl 


300         500          700          900         1100        1300 

Medium  (mosm.kg-1) 


Figure  6.  Variations  in  the  difference  between  the  osmotic  pressures  of  hemolymph  and  medium  according 
to  the  osmotic  pressure  ol  the  medium  in  zoeae  1-5,  megalopae.  and  first  crab  stage  of  Cancer  irroratiis  at 
15°C.  Each  point  represents  the  mean  value  of  determinations  from  9-15  animals  (5- 10  animals  in  extreme 
salinities)  with  95%  confidence  interval.  O O:  post-molt;  • •:  stage  C;  A  -  -  A:  premolt. 


hyper-osmoconform  in  salinities  that  they  normally  en- 
counter in  their  environment  (Charmantier  ct  ui,  1988). 
This  could  be  considered  an  adaptation  of  small  organisms 
to  planktonic  or  pelagic  life.  The  slight  positive  difference 
in  osmotic  pressure  between  hemolymph  and  medium 
maintains  an  osmotic  influx  of  water,  which  in  turn  favors 
the  turgescence  of  the  body  and  particularly  of  the  ex- 
tended appendages  and  exopodites  involved  in  the  buoy- 
ancy of  the  larvae.  In  a  few  zoeal  stages  of  C.  irroratiis, 
the  osmotic  pressure  of  hemolymph  is  affected  by  the 
molting  stage,  increasing  in  premolt  and  decreasing  in 
postmolt  but  much  less  regularly  than  in  other  species 
like  Rhithropanopeus  harrisii  (Kalber  and  Costlow,  1966), 
Cardisoma  giianhumi  (Kalber  and  Costlow,  1968),  Hom- 
arus  americanus  and  Penaeus  japonicus  (Charmantier  ct 
a/..  1988).  Like  zoeae,  megalopae  of  C.  irroratiis  hyper- 
osmoconform  in  all  media.  First  crab  stages  osmoconform 
in  seawater,  but  they  hyper-regulate  in  a  dilute  medium. 
Thus,  the  adult  type  of  osmoregulation  seems  to  be  ac- 
quired at  the  first  crab  stage.  However,  the  ability  to  hyper- 


regulate,  evaluated  by  the  difference  between  the  osmotic 
pressures  of  hemolymph  and  medium  in  a  dilute  medium, 
increases  with  size  in  adults:  in  a  medium  of  500 
mosm-kg"1,  17%»,  this  difference  was  55  ±  16 
mosm-kg"'  in  first  crab  stage,  55  ±  13  mosm-kg"1  in 
small  adults,  and  71  ±  7  mosm-kg"1  in  large  adults.  As 
in  C.  irroratiis,  zoeae  of  C.  borealis  hyper-osmoconform, 
while  adults  have  a  slight  hyper-isoregulation.  In  a  pre- 
liminary study.  Brown  and  Terwilliger  (1989)  found  that 
megalopae  and  first  crabs  of  C.  magister  were  weaker  os- 
moregulators  than  adults,  after  8  h  exposure  to  dilute  me- 
dia. A  comparison  with  our  results  is  difficult  due  to  the 
lack  of  numerical  data.  Additionally,  it  is  possible  that 
the  short  time  of  exposure  did  not  allow  for  complete 
osmotic  equilibration  in  adults. 

In  a  recent  study,  we  reviewed  the  evolution  of  os- 
moregulatory  abilities  that  have  been  described  for 
the  post-embryonic  development  of  decapod  crustaceans 
(Charmantier  el  ai.  1988).  Most  decapod  larvae  that  have 
been  studied,  including  zoeae  of  C.  irroratiis  and  C.  bo- 


132 


G.  CHARMANTIER  AND  M.  CHARMANTIER-DAURES 


60 


o> 

J* 

E 
o 

E 

D 
•O 


a 


Z3 


300  500  700  900          1100          1300 

Medium  (mosm.kg"1 ) 

Figure  7.  Variations  in  the  difference  between  the  osmotic  pressures 
of  hemolymph  and  medium  according  to  the  osmotic  pressure  of  the 
medium  in  zoeae  1-3  of  Cancer  borealis  at  I5°C.  Each  point  represents 
the  mean  value  of  determinations  from  10  animals  with  95%  confidence 
interval.  O O:  post-molt;  • •:  stage  C;  A  -  -  A:  premolt. 


realis,  are  hyper-osmoconformers  or  weak  regulators,  an 
exception  being  the  larvae  of  Macrobrachium  petersi 
(Read,  1 984),  which  are  confronted  with  very  low  salinities 
in  their  natural  environment  and  which  can  efficiently 
regulate  the  osmotic  concentration  of  their  hemolymph. 
During  or  after  the  larval  phase,  different  patterns  of  on- 
togeny of  osmoregulation  have  been  described,  which  we 
proposed  to  separate  into  three  groups  (Charmantier  et 
al.,  1988).  In  one  group  of  species,  osmoregulation  varies 
little  with  developmental  stage;  the  adults  of  these  species 
are  often  weak  regulators  or  osmoconformers,  like  He- 
patus  epheliticus  and  Libinia  emarginuta  (Kalber,  1970). 
In  M.  petersi  (Read,  1 984),  which  lives  in  variable  salinities 
due  to  its  migration,  the  adult  type  of  regulation  is  estab- 
lished as  early  as  the  first  larval  stage.  In  a  third  group  of 
species,  which  includes  Uca  subcylindrica  (Rabalais  and 
Cameron,  1 985),  Homarus  americanus,  Penaeus japon- 
icus  (Charmantier  et  at.,  \ 988),  and  Cancer  irroratus  (this 
study),  metamorphosis  marks  the  appearance  of  the  adult 
type  of  regulation.  In  other  species  that  do  not  still  fit  in 
those  three  categories,  in  which  "no  clear  trend  toward 
development  of  adult  osmoregulatory  patterns  toward  the 
end  of  larval  life"  was  found  (Foskett,  1 977),  this  could 
be  due  to  the  lack  of  information  about  the  osmoregu- 


latory capacity  of  early  post-metamorphic  stages.  For  ex- 
ample, in  Sesarmu  reticulatum  studied  by  Foskett,  1977, 
zoeae  and  megalopae  were  hyper-osmoconformers  and 
adults  were  hyper-hyporegulators,  but  early  crab  stages 
were  not  studied.  Foskett  stated:  ".  .  .  even  by  late  mega- 
lops  the  adult  osmoregulatory  response  is  still  not  attained. 
Examination  of  osmoregulation  in  early  juvenile  crab 
stages  may  reveal  osmoregulatory  responses  that  are  tran- 
sitional between  the  larval  and  adult  forms." 

The  few  studies  that  were  conducted  in  decapods 
throughout  the  post-embryonic  development,  including 
post-metamorphic  stages,  confirm  this  statement.  The 
transition  from  larval  to  adult  type  of  osmoregulation  may 
occur  rapidly  when  metamorphosis  itself  is  sudden,  as  in 
//.  americanus,  or  may  be  progressive  when  metamor- 
phosis is  spread  over  several  post-larval  stages  as  in  P. 
japonicus  (Charmantier  et  al..  1 988).  There  is  a  lack  of 
agreement  on  the  exact  timing  of  metamorphosis  in 
brachyurans:  it  has  been  located  either  at  the  molt  from 
megalopa  to  first  crab  stage  (Costlow.  1 968),  or  at  the 
molt  from  last  zoea  to  megalopa  (Felder  et  al..  1985). 
Actually,  most  morphological  changes  result  from  the 
molt  separating  the  last  zoea  and  the  megalopa  and,  in 
U.  subcylindrica,  the  type  of  osmoregulation  also  changes 
at  this  molt  ( Rabalais  and  Cameron,  1 985 ).  However,  this 
physiological  modification  occurs  only  after  the  molt  sep- 
arating the  megalopa  and  the  first  crab  stage  in  C.  irroratus 
(this  study),  and  possibly  in  5.  reticulatum  (Foskett,  1 977). 
Additionally,  as  stated  by  Felder  et  al..  (1985),  "in  almost 
all  the  Decapoda,  some  ontogenic  changes  in  locomotion, 
feeding,  and  habitat  coincide  with  early  postlarval 
growth."  Thus,  in  our  opinion,  metamorphosis  in  brach- 
yurans requires  two  molts  to  be  completed  and  the  mega- 
lopa, while  clearly  postlarval,  is  a  transitional  stage  be- 
tween the  larvae  and  the  postmetamorphic  stages  starting 
with  the  first  crab  stage. 

In  summary,  studies  conducted  on  the  species  of  the 
third  group  of  decapods  cited  above  demonstrate  that  the 
completion  of  metamorphosis  yields  a  change  to  the  adult 
type  of  osmoregulation.  We  propose  the  hypothesis  that, 
in  most  species  in  which  larvae  are  hyper-osmoconformers 
or  weak  regulators  and  adults  are  efficient  osmoregulators, 
the  transition  from  the  larval  type  to  the  adult  type  of 
osmoregulation  occurs  at  metamorphosis.  More  generally, 
metamorphosis  can  be  considered  a  combination  of  mor- 
phological, ecological,  behavioral,  and  physiological 
changes  (Costlow,  1 968;  Charmantier  et  al..  I984b). 

Relation  between  osmoregulation  and  salinity  tolerance 

In  C.  irroratus  and  C.  borealis.  zoeae  are  weak  regu- 
lators and  their  salinity  tolerance  is  comparatively  mod- 
erate or  low.  In  megalopae  of  C.  irroratus,  salinity  toler- 
ance is  minimum  just  before  the  pattern  of  osmoregula- 


ONTOGENY  OF  CANCER  OSMOREGULATION 


133 


tion  changes.  Under  natural  conditions,  developing 
brachyurans  are  known  to  settle  on  the  bottom  during 
the  megalopal  stage,  usually  near  the  shores  (Sandifer, 
1973),  i.e..  in  an  environment  subjected  to  possible  vari- 
ations of  salinity.  The  limited  salinity  tolerance  of  mega- 
lopae  could  cause  high  rates  of  mortality,  which  have  been 
noted  in  different  species  of  Cancer,  at  least  under  culture 
conditions  (Charmantier-Daures  and  Charmantier,  1991). 
Unsufficient  number  of  available  animals  prevented  us 
from  determining  the  salinity  tolerance  of  early  crab 
stages,  in  which  the  pattern  of  osmoregulation  has 
changed.  We  may  suppose  that  their  salinity  tolerance 
has  correlatively  increased.  In  adults  of  both  species,  the 
ability  to  osmoregulate  is  higher  and  so  are  their  salinity 
tolerances.  Compared  to  C.  borealis,  adult  C.  irroratm 
are  stronger  hyper-regulators  in  dilute  media  and  are  more 
tolerant  to  low  salinity.  Thus,  as  previously  noted  in  Mac- 
robrachiwn  petersi  (Read,  1984),  Uca  subcylindriai  (Ra- 
balais  and  Cameron,  1985),  Homarus  americanus  and 
Penaeus  japonicus  (Charmantier  et  til.,  1988),  there  is  a 
strong  correlation  between  increased  ability  to  osmoregu- 
late and  improved  salinity  tolerance. 

Acknowledgments 

Part  of  this  study  was  supported  by  a  grant  from  NATO. 
We  thank  Dr.  D.  E.  Aiken  for  providing  lab  space  and 
facilities,  and  Ross  Chandler,  Jay  Parsons,  David  Robi- 
chaud,  and  Wilfred  Young-Lai  for  their  help  in  capturing 
and  rearing  crabs. 

Literature  Cited 

Bigford,  T.  E.  1979.  Synopsis  of  biological  data  on  the  Rock  Crah. 
Cancer  irroralns  Say.  NOAA  Techn  Rep  NMFS  426.  FAO  Synopsis 
n°  123,  26  pp. 

Brown,  A.  C.,  and  N.  B.  Terwilliger.  1989.  Developmental  changes  in 
osmotic  and  ionic  regulation  in  the  Dungeness  crab,  Cancer  magister. 
Am  /<«)/.  29:  156A. 

Cantelmo,  A.  C.,  F.  R.  Cantelmo,  and  D.  M.  Langsam. 
1975.  Osmoregulatory  ability  of  the  rock  crab.  Cancer  irroralus. 
under  osmotic  stress.  Comp.  Biochem  Physiol.  51A:  537-542. 

Charmantier,  G.  1986.  Variation  des  capacites  osmoregulatnces  au 
cours  du  developpement  post-embryonnaire  de  Penaeus  laponicus 
Bate,  1888  (Crustacea  Decapoda).  C  R  AcaiL  Sci.  Paris  303:  217- 
222 

Charmantier,  G.,  M.  Charmantier-Daures,  and  D.  E.  Aiken. 
1984a.  Variation  des  capacites  osmoregulatrices  des  larves  et  post- 
larves  de  Homarus  americanus  H.  Milne-Edwards.  1 837  (Crustacea, 
Decapoda).  C.  R  Acad.  Sci.  Pans  299:  863-866. 

Charmantier,  G.,  M.  Charmantier-Daures,  and  D.  E.  Aiken. 
1984b.  Neuroendocnne  control  ol'hydromineral  regulation  in  the 
American  lobster  Homarus  americanus  H.  Milne-Edwards,  1837 
(Crustacea,  Decapoda).  2.  Larval  and  post-larval  stages.  Gen.  Comp 
Endocrmol.  54:  20-34. 

Charmantier,  G.,  M.  Charmantier-Daures,  N.  Bouaricha,  P.  Thuet, 
D.  E.  Aiken,  and  J.-P.  Trilles.  1988.  Ontogeny  of  osmoregulation 
and  salinity  tolerance  in  two  decapod  crustaceans:  Homarus  amer- 
icanus and  Penaeus  japonicus.  Biol  Bull  175:  102-110. 


Charmantier,  G.,  M.  Charmantier-Daures,  N.  Bouaricha,  P.  I  hurt. 
J.-P.  Trilles,  and  D.  K.  Aiken.  1989.  Ontogeny  of  osmoregulation 
and  salinity  tolerance  in  Homarus  americanus,  Penaeus  laponicus 
and  Cancer  irroratus  Am.  /<><>/.  29:  155A. 

Charmantier-Daures,  M.,  and  G.  Charmantier.  1991.  Mass-culture  of 
Cancer  irroralus  larvae  (Crustacea,  Decapoda):  adaptation  of  a  flow- 
through  sea  water  system.  Aquaculture. 

Costlow,  J.  D.,  Jr.  1968.  Metamorphosis  in  crustaceans.  Pp.  3-41  in 
Metamorphosis.  W.  Etkin  and  L.  I.  Gilbert,  eds.  Appleton-Century- 
Crofts.  New  York. 

Drach,  P.  1939.  Mue  et  cycle  d'intermue  chez  les  Crustaces  Decapodes. 
Ann  lust  Oceanogr..  Monaco  \9:  103-341. 

Engelhardt,  F.  R.,  and  P.  A.  Dehnel.  1973.  Ionic  regulation  in  the 
Pacific  edible  crab.  Cancer  magister  (Dana).  Can.  J  7.ool.  51:  735- 
743. 

Felder.  J.  M.,  D.  L.  Felder,  and  S.  C.  Hand.  1986.  Ontogeny  of  os- 
moregulation in  the  estuarine  ghost  shrimp  Callumassa  iamaicen.se 
var.  louisianensis  Schmitt  (Decapoda.  Thalassinidea).  J  Exp.  Mar 
Biol  Ecol.  99:91-105. 

Felder,  D.  L.,  J.  \V.  Martin,  and  J.  \V.  Goy.  1985.  Patterns  in  early 
postlarval  development  of  decapods.  Pp.  163-225  in  Larval  Growth, 
A.  M.  Wenner,  ed.  A.  A.  Balkema.  Rotterdam.  Boston. 

Finney,  D.  J.  1962.  Probit  Analysis  1  Statistical  Treatment  of  the 
Sigmoid  Response  Curve  Second  Edition.  Cambridge  Univ.  Press, 
London. 

Foskett,  J.  K.  1977.  Osmoregulation  in  the  larvae  and  adults  of  the 
grapsid  crab  Sesarma  rclicutalum  Say.  Biol  Bull  153:  505-526. 

Hughes,  J.  T.,  R.  A.  Shleser,  and  G.  Tchobanoglous.  1974.  A  rearing 
tank  for  lobster  larvae  and  other  aquatic  species.  Progr.  Fish.  Cult. 
36:  129-132. 

Hunter,  K.  C.,  and  P.  P.  Rudy  Jr.  1975.  Osmotic  and  ionic  regulation 
in  the  Dungeness  crab.  Cancer  magisler  Dana.  Comp  Biochem 
Physiol.  51  A:  439-447. 

Johns,  D.  M.  1981.  Physiological  studies  on  Cancer  irroralus  larvae. 
I.  Effects  of  temperature  and  salinity  on  survival,  development  rate 
and  size.  Mar.  Ecol.  Progr.  Ser.  5:  75-83. 

Jones,  L.  L.  1941.  Osmotic  regulation  in  several  crabs  of  the  Pacific 
coast  of  North  America.  J  Cell.  Comp.  Physiol.  18:  79-92. 

Kalber,  F.  A.  1970.  Osmoregulation  in  decapod  larvae  as  a  consider- 
ation in  culture  techniques.  Helgoi.  M'I.V.V.  Mcercsuntcrs.  20:  697- 
706. 

kalber,  F.  A.,  and  J.  D.  Costlow.  1966.  The  ontogeny  of  osmoregulation 
and  its  neurosecretory  control  in  the  decapod  crustacean,  Rhithro- 
panopeus  harrisii.  Am.  Zool.  6:  221-229. 

Kalber,  F.  A.,  and  J.  D.  Costlow.  1968.  Osmoregulation  in  larvae  of 
the  land  crab,  Cardisoma  guanhumi  Latreille.  Am.  Zool.  8: 4 1 1  -4 1 6. 

I  it  lulu  Id.  J.  T.,  and  F.  Wilcoxon.  1949.  The  reliability  of  graphic  es- 
timates of  relative  potency  from  dose-percent  effect  curves.  J  Phar- 
macol  E.\p  Tlieor.  108:  18-25. 

Lieberman,  H.  R.  1983.  Estimating  LD  50  using  the  probit  technique: 
a  basic  computer  program.  Drug  Chem  Toxicol.  6:  111-116. 

Mantel,  L.  H.,  and  L.  L.  Farmer.  1983.  Osmotic  and  ionic  regulation. 
Pp  53-161  in  The  Biology  of  Crustacea.  I'ol.  5.  Internal  Anatomy 
and  Physiological  Regulation,  L.  H.  Mantel,  ed.  Academic  Press, 
New  York. 

McCluskey,  W.  J.  1975.  The  effects  of  hyposaline  shocks  on  two  species 
of  Cancer  Unpubl.  manuscr.,  15  p.  Narragansett  Marine  Laboratory, 
University  of  Rhode  Island,  Narragansett,  RI  02882. 

Neufeld,  G.  J.,  and  J.  B.  Pritchard.  1979.  Osmoregulation  and  gill 
Na+,  K1  ATPase  in  the  rock  crab.  Cancer  irroratus:  response  to  DDT. 
Comp.  Biochem  Physiol.  62C:  165-172. 

Rabalais,  N.  N.,  and  J.  N.  Cameron.  1985.  The  effects  of  factors  im- 
portant in  semi-arid  environments  on  the  early  development  of  Uca 
subcylindrica  Biol.  Bull.  168:  147-160. 


134 


G.  CHARMANTIER  AND  M.  CHARMANTIER-DAURES 


Read,  CJ.  II.  L.  1984.  Intraspecific  variation  in  the  osmoregulatory  ca- 
pacity of  larval,  post-larval,  juvenile  and  adult  Macrnhrachiiim  petersi 
(Hilgendorf).  Comp.  Biochem.  Physiol.  78A:  501-506. 

Sandifer,  P.  A.  1973.  Distribution  and  abundance  of  decapod  crusta- 
cean larvae  in  the  York  River  estuary  and  adjacent  Chesapeake  Bay, 
Virginia.  1968-1969.  Che.ianciike  Sci.  14:  235-257. 

Sastry,  A.  N.  1970.  Culture  of  brachyuran  crab  larvae  using  a  re-cir- 
culating sea  water  system  in  the  laboratory,  llelgol  II  V.v.v  Meeresun- 
lers,  20:406-416. 

Sastry,  A.  N.  I977a.  The  larval  development  of  the  rock  crab.  Cancer 
irruralus  Say,  1817.  under  laboratory'  conditions  (Decapoda  Brachy- 
ura).  Crustaceana  32:  155-168. 

Sastry1-  A.  N.  1977b.  The  larval  development  of  the  Jonah  crab.  Cancer 
borealis  Stimpson.  1859.  under  laboratory  conditions  (Decapoda 
Brachyura).  Crustaceana  32:  290-303. 


Sastry,  A.  N.,  and  J.  F.  McCarthy.  1973.  Diversity  in  metabolic  ad- 
aptation of  pelagic  larval  stages  of  two  sympatnc  species  of  brachyuran 
crabs.  Nelherl.  J  Sea  Res  7:  434-446. 

Sprague,  J.  B.  1969.  Measurement  of  pollutant  toxicity  to  fish.  I. 
Bioassay  methods  for  acute  toxicity.  Water  Res  3:  793-821. 

Thurberg,  F.  P.,  M.  A.  Dawson,  and  R.  S.  Collier.  1973.  Effects  of 
copper  and  cadmium  on  osmoregulation  and  oxygen  consumption 
in  two  species  of  estuarine  crabs.  Mar.  Bio/.  23:  171-175. 

\\  .HIM >n.  S..  A.  Pequeux,  and  R.  Gilles.  1983.  Osmoregulation  in  the 
stone  crab  Cancer  pagurtix.  Mar.  Bio/.  Lett.  4:  321-330. 

Young,  A.  M.  1979.  Osmoregulation  in  larvae  of  the  striped  hermit 
crab  Clibanarius  villains  (Bosc)  (Decapoda:  Anomoura,  Diogenidae). 
Est.  Coast.  Mar  Sci.  9:  595-601. 

/itko,  Y.  1982.  Letcur,  the  lethality  curve  program.  Can.  Tech.  Rep 
Fish.  A(/tial.  Sci.  1134:  10  pp. 


Reference:  Biol  Bull  180:  L  35- 153.  (February, 


Sulfide-Driven  Autotrophic  Balance  in  the  Bacterial 
Symbiont-Containing  Hydrothermal  Vent  Tubeworm, 

Riftia  pachyptila  Jones 

J.  J.  CHILDRESS1.  C.  R.  FISHER-.  J.  A.  FAVUZZI1,  R.  E.  KOCHEVAR1. 
N.  K.  SANDERS3,  AND  A.   M.  ALAYSE4 

*  Department  of  Biological  Sciences  and  Marine  Science  Institute.  University  of  California,  Santa 

Barbara,  California  93106,  ^Department  of  Biological  Sciences.  Pennsylvania  State  University. 

University  Park.  Pennsylvania  16802.  'Bam/ielil  Marine  Station,  Bamfield.  British  Coiimhia. 

Canada  I  OR  I  BO.  and4  Department  Environment  Profond.  1FREMER. 

Centre  de  Brest,  B.  P.  70-29263  Plouiane.  France 


Abstract.  Hydrothermal  vent  tubeworms,  Riftia  pa- 
chyptila Jones,  were  maintained  alive  and  studied  on 
board  ship  using  flow-through  pressure  aquaria.  Simul- 
taneous measurements  of  O2,  SCO:,  -H2S  fluxes  showed 
that  the  intact  symbioses  reach  maximum  rates  of  uptake 
of  2CO2  ( > 2  Mmole  g~ '  h~ ' )  at  about  90  \iM  2H2S.  Mea- 
surements were  made  of  hemolymph  and  coelomic  fluid 
2CO2.  2H2S,  thiosulfate,  pH,  and  hemoglobin  concen- 
trations in  worms  kept  under  various  conditions  of  O2 
and  2H2S.  Normal  hemolymph  pH  appears  to  be  about 
7.5  and  is  not  affected  by  2H2S  and  2CO2  concentrations 
within  the  ranges  observed.  We  conclude  that  Riftia  is 
specialized  to  provide  sulfide  to  its  symbionts  with  min- 
imal interaction  of  sulfide  with  the  animal  metabolism. 
The  uptake  of  sulfide  is  apparently  by  diffusion  into  the 
hemolymph,  facilitated  by  the  sulfide-binding  properties 
of  the  hemoglobins.  Both  2CO2  and  PCo,  are  elevated  in 
the  hemolymph  above  their  levels  in  the  medium,  al- 
though they  are  reduced  under  autotrophic  conditions. 
Thus  inorganic  carbon  is  apparently  concentrated  from 
the  medium  into  the  hemolymph  by  an  unknown  mech- 
anism. 

Introduction 

The  giant  hydrothermal  vent  tubeworm,  Riftia  pa- 
chyptila Jones,  is  perhaps  the  most  distinctive  of  the  an- 
imals living  around  the  deep-sea  hydrothermal  vents.  Like 

Received  10  September  1990;  accepted  24  November  1990. 


all  vestimentiferan  tubeworms,  adults  of  this  species  lack 
a  mouth  and  a  gut  (Jones,  1981,  1988;  Jones  and  Gardiner, 
1988;  Southward,  1988).  The  adult  worms  appear  to  de- 
rive their  nutritional  needs  from  the  large  population  of 
sulfur-oxidizing  chemolithoautotrophic  bacterial  sym- 
bionts that  live  in  cells  within  a  specialized  organ — the 
trophosome — in  their  trunk  (Cavanaugh  el  al..  1981;  Fel- 
beck,  198 1 ).  The  trophosome  is  a  highly  vascularized  or- 
gan lying  between  two  coelomic  cavities  that  contain  a 
hemoglobin-rich  fluid  (Jones,  1988).  This  anatomy  re- 
quires that  the  animal  supply  the  needs  of  the  symbionts 
through  its  circulatory  system  (Arp  et  al..  1985;  Felbeck 
and  Childress.  1988).  Because  these  symbionts  are  sulfide- 
oxidizing  autotrophs  (Felbeck,  1981;  Belkin  et  al..  1986; 
Fisher  et  al..  1989),  the  worm  must  take  up  sulfide,  oxygen, 
and  carbon  dioxide  from  the  medium  and  transport  them 
to  the  symbionts.  These  substances  can  be  taken  up  from 
the  water  by  the  large  obturacular  plume,  a  highly  vas- 
cularized organ  that  has  a  large  surface  area  and  brings 
the  hemolymph  very  close  to  the  surrounding  water  (Arp 
et  al..  1985;  Jones,  1988).  The  hemolymph  and  the  coe- 
lomic fluid  both  have  abundant  extracellular  hemoglobins 
which  are  believed  to  play  a  key  role  in  the  transport  of 
all  three  of  these  metabolites  (Childress  et  al..  1984;  Arp 
et  al..  1985). 

Two  hemoglobins  are  found  in  the  extracellular  fluids 
of  these  worms.  One  has  a  molecular  weight  of  about  1.7 
X  106  Mrand  is  found  primarily  in  the  hemolymph,  while 
the  second  is  smaller  (0.4  X  106  Mr)  and  is  found  in  both 
the  coelomic  and  vascular  compartments  (Terwilliger  et 


135 


136 


J    J    CHILDRESS  ET  AL 


a/..  1980;  Arp  and  Childress,  1981;  Terwilliger  and  Ter- 
williger,  1985;  Arp  el  ai.  1987).  Both  hemoglobins  bind 
oxygen  and  sulfide  reversibly  with  a  high  affinity  (Arp  and 
Childress,  1981,  1983;  Childress  et  ai,  1984;  Arp  el  ai. 
1987;  Fisher  et  ai,  1988a).  The  sulfide  binding  does  not 
affect  the  simultaneous  binding  of  oxygen,  and  appears 
to  occur  at  a  site  removed  from  the  heme  (Childress  et 
ai.  1984;  Arp  et  ai.  1987).  When  sulfide  and  oxygen  are 
below  saturation  in  the  hemolymph,  their  normally  rapid, 
spontaneous  reaction  is  suppressed  (Fisher  and  Childress, 
1984).  Further,  the  hemoglobin  can  protect  the  animal 
tissues  from  sulfide  toxicity  by  binding  the  sulfide  with  a 
higher  affinity  than  does  the  site  of  toxic  effects,  cyto- 
chrome-c-oxidase  (Powell  and  Somero,  1983,  1986).  The 
hemoglobin  does,  however,  release  sulfide  to  the  sym- 
bionts  while  simultaneously  protecting  them  from  sulfide 
toxicity  by  holding  free  sulfide  concentrations  down 
(Fisher  and  Childress,  1984;  Fisher  el  ai.  1988a,  1989). 
The  hemoglobins  also  buffer  the  hemolymph  for  carbon 
dioxide  transport  (Childress  et  ai,  1984).  Thus,  the  he- 
molymph apparently  has  the  properties  required  to  take 
oxygen,  carbon  dioxide,  and  sulfide  from  the  medium 
and  to  transport  them  to  the  endosymbionts. 

Most  of  the  experiments  described  in  this  paper  were 
performed  to  test  the  role  of  the  hemolymph  in  gas  uptake 
and  transport  in  intact,  living  Riftiapachyptila  individuals. 
In  particular,  we  were  concerned  with  demonstrating  the 
continuous  uptake  and  oxidation  of  sulfide  by  the  intact 
organisms,  evaluating  the  role  of  the  hemoglobins  in  con- 
centrating sulfide  from  the  medium,  looking  for  the  pos- 
sible roles  of  other  forms  of  sulfur  in  the  symbiosis,  ex- 
amining the  impact  of  sulfide  and  symbiont  autotrophy 
on  internal  CO2  pools  and  pH,  and  observing  the  pattern 
of  exchange  of  gases  between  the  coelomic  fluid  and  the 
hemolymph. 

A  consistent  chemical  terminology  will  be  used 
throughout  this  paper.  Sulfide  and  inorganic  carbon  refer 
to  these  substances  without  specifying  the  chemical  species 
involved.  2H:S  and  2CO2  refer  to  the  amounts  of  these 
gases  analyzed  from  acidified  samples  using  the  analytical 
methods  described  below.  They  are  measures  of  the  sum 
of  the  various  chemical  forms  in  which  these  substances 
are  found.  H:S,  HS~,  S2~,  S°,  CO2,  HCO,  and  any  other 
chemical  formulae  refer  only  to  the  chemical  species 
symbolized.  "Free"  refers  to  that  fraction  of  a  substance 
in  the  body  fluids  that  is  not  bound  to  the  hemoglobins. 

Materials  and  Methods 

The  tubeworms  used  in  these  studies  were  collected 
from  depths  of  about  2600  m  at  sites  on  the  Galapagos 
Rift  (00°48.247'N,  86°  1 3.478^)  and  the  East  Pacific  Rise 
(12°48'N,  108°57'W)  by  deep  submersibles  (Alvin  at  the 
Galapagos  Rift  site  and  Nautile  at  the  East  Pacific  Rise 


site).  Both  submersibles  pulled  the  worms  off  the  rocks 
using  their  manipulators,  placed  them  in  thermally  in- 
sulated containers,  and  brought  them  to  the  surface  about 
2  to  8  h  after  capture.  Once  at  the  surface  the  worms  were 
quickly  transferred  to  cold  seawater  (7°C)  where  undam- 
aged worms  were  set  apart  for  the  whole  animal  experi- 
ments described  here.  The  worms  chosen  were  then  care- 
fully removed  from  their  natural  tubes  and  placed  in 
straight  plastic  tubes  of  appropriate  size  so  they  could  be 
fitted  into  the  pressure  vessels  necessary  for  their  mainte- 
nance. They  were  then  quickly  placed  in  pressure  aquaria. 

The  worms  were  routinely  maintained  in  flowing-water 
pressure  aquaria  (Quetin  and  Childress,  1980)  at  200  atm 
pressure,  8°C,  and  more  than  100  nAI  O2.  Water  was 
pumped  through  these  stainless  steel  pressure  vessels  at 
about  12  1/h.  Previous  studies  (Childress  et  ai.  1984)  and 
preliminary  observations  during  this  study  indicate  that 
although  the  worms  live  at  a  hydrostatic  pressure  of  about 
260  atm  at  these  sites,  they  are  able  to  survive  and  display 
apparently  "normal"  behavior  at  pressures  as  low  as  about 
100  atm.  The  symbionts  themselves  do  not  show  signif- 
icant effects  of  pressure  on  carbon  fixation  rates  within 
the  pressure  range  used  here  (Fisher  et  ai,  1989).  In  the 
present  study  pressures  as  low  as  120  atm  were  used  in 
some  experiments,  but  the  experience  cited  above  suggests 
that  these  lower  pressures  should  have  little  effect  on  the 
results. 

Studies  involving  the  maintenance  of  the  worms  at 
known  sulfide  concentrations  were  carried  out  in  flowing- 
water  aquaria  (120  atm,  about  4  1/h)  using  transparent 
acrylic  pressure  vessels  (Quetin  and  Childress,  1980),  al- 
lowing the  activity  of  the  worms  to  be  observed  during 
the  experiments.  Anaerobic  sulfide  stock  solution  (5  or 
10  mM  sodium  sulfide  in  seawater  at  ph  7.0  or  7.5)  was 
added  continuously  at  the  intakes  of  the  pressure  pumps 
with  low  pressure  metering  pumps  to  achieve  stable  sulfide 
concentrations  in  the  pressure  vessels.  The  effluent  water 
from  the  vessels  was  periodically  sampled  with  a  0.5  ml 
glass  syringe,  and  the  gases  were  analyzed  by  gas  chro- 
matography  (Childress  et  ai.  1984).  The  pH  of  the  effluent 
water  was  measured  with  a  double  junction  electrode  and 
was  between  7.  and  8.1,  depending  on  the  experiment. 

Metabolism  measurements 

Measurements  of  whole  animal  metabolism  were  made 
in  a  flowing  water  system  similar  to  that  used  by  Anderson 
et  ai.  (1987),  but  adapted  for  use  at  the  high  pressures 
required  for  the  survival  of  the  worms.  The  system 
pumped  seawater  through  the  respirometer  chambers  us- 
ing HPLC  pressure  pumps  with  small  acrylic  pressure 
vessels  as  respirometer  chambers.  The  water  in  this  system 
was  first  passed  through  a  series  of  filters  (5.0  and  0.2  yum) 
and  a  UV  sterilizer.  It  was  then  continuously  mixed  by 


AUTOTROPHIC  FUNCTION  IN  RlH'l.l 


137 


means  of  metering  pumps  with  an  antibiotic  solution  to 
achieve  a  final  concentration  of  150  mg  penicillin-G  per 
liter  and  with  a  sulfide  solution  (pH  7.5  in  seawater)  to 
achieve  the  desired  sulfide  concentration.  It  then  went  to 
a  vertically  oriented  column  measuring  1  X  0. 1  m,  with 
the  seawater  entering  at  the  top  and  exiting  near  the  bot- 
tom. The  pH  of  the  water  in  the  column  was  maintained 
at  7.5  by  a  pH  controller  that  pumped  1  Af  acid  (HCL) 
or  base  (NaOH)  into  the  column.  Oxygen  and  N2  bubbled 
via  the  bottom  of  the  column  mixed  the  water  in  the 
column  while  maintaining  the  desired  O2  concentration. 
The  water  was  then  pumped  through  the  respirometer 
chambers  to  a  gas  chromatograph  for  analysis.  Two  res- 
pirometer streams  were  continuously  used  in  these  mea- 
surements, one  with  animals  in  the  respirometer  chamber 
and  the  other  an  identical  system  without  animals,  which 
served  as  a  control  for  spontaneous  oxidation  of  sulfide. 
Fluxes  of  the  measured  gases  due  to  the  animals  were 
calculated  from  the  differences  in  gas  concentrations  in 
the  water  exiting  the  experimental  and  control  chambers. 
These  experiments  were  carried  out  at  1 30  atm  hydrostatic 
pressure. 

Ammonium  flux  was  measured  for  several  worms  while 
they  were  in  the  respirometer  system  described  above. 
The  ammonium  concentrations  in  the  effluents  from  the 
two  chambers  were  measured  by  flow  injection  analysis 
(Willason  and  Johnson,  1986). 

Dissection  procedure 

Worms  were  dissected  so  that  samples  of  hemolymph, 
coelomic  fluid,  and  trophosome  could  be  obtained  for 
further  analysis.  Worms  to  be  sacrificed  were  quickly  re- 
moved from  the  pressure  aquaria  and  the  plastic  tubes 
and  then  stretched  out  in  a  dissecting  tray.  The  body  wall 
below  the  vestimentum  was  carefully  slit  for  a  few  cen- 
timeters parallel  to  the  main  axis  of  the  worm  on  the 
ventral  side.  A  sample  of  coelomic  fluid  (1-5  ml)  was 
quickly  drawn,  with  a  blunt  needle,  from  the  pool  of  this 
fluid  in  the  coelomic  space  and  placed  on  ice.  Subsamples 
for  the  various  analyses  were  quickly  taken.  The  remaining 
coelomic  fluid  was  then  drained  from  the  worm,  and  a 
1-ml  syringe  with  a  30-ga  needle  was  used  to  remove  he- 
molymph from  the  major  dorsal  vessel  leading  from  the 
trophosome  to  the  plume  of  the  worm.  Aliquots  of  this 
post-trophosome  (pre-branchial)  hemolymph  sample  were 
quickly  taken  for  the  various  analyses.  Samples  of  tro- 
phosome tissue  were  also  frozen  for  later  analysis  of  ele- 
mental sulfur.  If  the  trophosome  appeared  "unhealthy" 
[the  pinkish  appearance  correlated  with  lack  of  CO:  fix- 
ation in  trophosome  preparations  (Fisher  et  a/.,  1989), 
occurred  in  7  of  the  50  animals  used]  for  an  individual 
worm,  the  data  from  that  worm  were  excluded  from  fur- 
ther consideration.  These  unhealthy  worms  were  always 
characterized  by  low  (<7.0)  hemolymph  pH  values. 


Analytical  methods 


Gas  chromatographic  methods  similar  to  those  de- 
scribed by  Childress  el  al.  (1984)  were  used  to  analyze 
gases  in  body  fluids  and  seawater.  Briefly,  water  samples 
were  acidified  with  phosphoric  acid,  and  gases  were 
stripped  from  them  using  a  glass  and  teflon  extractor,  in- 
line with  a  thermal  conductivity  gas  chromatograph.  This 
system  allowed  the  analysis  of  the  O:,  CO:,  H^S,  N-,, 
CH4,  and  CO  concentrations  in  fluid  samples  of  0.2  to 
1 .0  ml.  The  limit  of  sensitivity  for  these  gases  was  between 
1  and  20  nM.  depending  on  the  gas  and  the  sample  size. 
Throughout  this  paper,  the  terms  2H;S  and  SCO,  refer 
to  the  amounts  measured  using  this  analytical  method 
without  regard  for  the  chemical  species  present  at  the  very 
different  pH  values  and  conditions  in  the  worms. 

To  measure  pH,  a  sample  of  hemolymph  or  coelomic 
fluid  was  drawn  from  an  animal  with  a  syringe.  The  dead 
space  of  the  syringe  was  filled  with  blood  by  drawing  a 
small  amount  of  sample  into  the  syringe  and  then  expel- 
ling the  air  and  excess  blood  before  drawing  the  sample 
for  analysis.  Without  air  exposure,  the  sample  was  im- 
mediately injected  into  a  Radiometer  glass  capillary  elec- 
trode (Radiometer  America  G298A)  used  in  conjunction 
with  a  reference  electrode  (Radiometer  K171)  in  a  water 
jacketed  chamber.  Precision  buffers  (Radiometer  SI 500 
&  S1510)  were  used  to  calibrate  the  electrode. 

The  abundances  of  the  two  hemoglobins  in  the  he- 
molymph were  quantified  by  separating  them  by  HPLC 
gel  filtration  and  measuring  the  absorbance  as  they  eluted 
from  the  column  (Arp  et  al.,  1987).  A  TSK-50  column, 
7.5  mm  in  diameter  and  300  mm  long,  was  used  with  a 
TSK  guard  column  (7.5  mm  by  75  mm).  The  eluent  was 
a  citric  acid/phosphate  buffer  ( 1 .63  g  citric  acid  and  26. 1 7 
g  KH2PO4/1)  at  pH  7.5,  pumped  at  0.3  ml/min  at  5°C. 
The  run  time  was  about  40  min,  and  an  undiluted  1-yul 
sample  was  used.  The  absorbance  was  measured  at  4 1 5 
nm  as  the  eluent  left  the  column. 

Determinations  of  thiosulfate  and  other  unbound  thiols 
in  the  body  fluids  were  made  by  HPLC  analysis  of  samples 
derivatized  by  monobromobimane  using  the  methods  of 
Newton  et  al.  (1981)  and  Fahey  et  al.  (1983)  as  modified 
by  Vetter  et  al.  (1989).  Derivatives  were  separated  on  a 
15  cm  C- 18  reversed  phase  column  and  detected  using  a 
235  nm  filter  for  excitation  and  a  442  nm  filter  for  detec- 
tion of  fluorescence.  The  eluent  flow  rate  was  1.5  ml  per 
min,  using  an  increasing  hydrophobic  gradient  of  HPLC 
grade  methanol  and  2%  acetic  acid,  starting  at  10%  meth- 
anol  and  increasing  to  100%  during  the  run. 

Elemental  sulfur  in  the  extracts  was  quantified  by  gas 
chromatography  according  to  the  method  of  Richard  et 
al.  (1977)  as  modified  by  Fisher  et  al.  (1988b).  Pieces  of 
tissue  (0.5-2.0  g  wet  weight)  were  dried  for  18  h  in  a  100°C 
drying  oven,  and  then  extracted  for  24  h  with  cyclohexane 


138 


J.  J.  CHILDRESS  ET  AL. 


in  a  micro-Soxhlet  apparatus.  The  extracts  were  "cleaned 
up"  by  passing  them  through  a  fluorosil  column  to  remove 
lipids,  and  concentrated  by  evaporation.  The  injector 
temperature  was  240°C,  and  the  initial  column  temper- 
ature was  150°C,  programmed  to  220°C  during  the  sep- 
aration. A  six  foot  (1.8  m)  glass  column  with  a  2  mm 
bore,  packed  with  5%  SP2401  on  100/120  mesh  Supel- 
coport,  was  used  to  separate  sulfur.  The  sulfur  was  detected 
and  quantified  using  a  thermal  conductivity  detector.  The 
detection  limit  for  elemental  sulfur  was  ca.  0.001%  of  the 
dry  weight  of  the  sample  (depending  somewhat  on  sample 
size).  The  identity  of  the  separated  sulfur  was  confirmed 
by  the  distinctive  smell  of  sulfur  vapor  coming  out  of  the 
gas  chromatograph  detector  at  the  time  of  the  putative 
sulfur  peak. 

Estimation  of  free  ~ZH:S  and  H}S 

Because  2H2S,  pH,  and  hemoglobin  contents  were 
measured,  it  was  possible,  using  previously  published 
data,  to  estimate  the  concentration  of  free  (unbound) 
2H2S  as  well  as  the  various  species  of  sulfide.  Free  2H2S 
was  estimated  by  using  the  Hill  equation  describing  the 
relationship  between  fractional  saturation  and  free  sul- 
fide measured  at  6°C,  pH  7.5  in  a  mixture  of  coelomic 
fluid  and  hemolymph  (Fisher  et  ill.,  1988a):  In  [%  sat- 
uration/(100  -  %  saturation)]  =  0.737(ln  free  2H2S 
fiM)-  1.778. 

To  use  this  equation,  the  capacity  of  each  fluid  sample 
to  bind  sulfide  was  estimated  by  multiplying  the  small 
hemoglobin  aggregate  concentration  by  one  sulnde/heme 
and  the  large  aggregate  concentration  by  three  sulndes/ 
heme.  These  estimates  were  derived  from  a  multiple 
regression  of  the  sulfide  concentrations  in  nine  coelomic 
fluid  samples  dialyzed  at  saturating  sulfide  concentrations 
against  the  concentrations  of  the  two  aggregates  in  those 
samples  (data  from  Arp,  1987).  This  regression  had  an  r2 
of  0.97  and  gave  coefficients  of  0.90  ±  0.27  (95%  C.  I.) 
and  2.97  ±  0.86,  respectively,  for  the  two  hemoglobins. 
From  the  estimated  capacity  for  binding  sulfide  and  the 
measured  ZH2S  in  the  fluid,  the  percent  saturation  was 
approximated,  and  the  above  equation  was  solved  for  free 
sulfide.  This  approximation  of  free  sulfide  was  then  used 
with  the  estimated  sulfide  binding  capacity  [%  saturation 
=  100  (bound  sulfide/binding  capacity)]  in  the  Hill  equa- 
tion to  estimate  the  sulfide  bound  to  the  hemoglobin.  This 
procedure  was  then  carried  through  several  iterations  until 
the  estimates  converged  on  a  single  value  for  free  sulfide. 
This  value  was  then  used  to  calculate  the  percentage  sat- 
uration of  the  fluid. 

The  free  H2S  in  each  sample  was  calculated  from  the 
free  2H2S  using  a  pK,  value  (8°C,  35%o  and  120  atm)  of 


6.784  (Millero.  1986;  Millero  et  ai.  1988)  and  the  pH 
measured  in  that  particular  sample. 

Estimation  ofPCo: 

Because  2CO:.  pH,  and  hemoglobin  contents  were 
measured,  it  was  possible,  using  previously  published  data, 
to  estimate  the  PCo,  in  the  fluids.  The  data  relating  pH. 
2CO:,  and  PCO;  in  Rifiia  coelomic  fluid  at  10°C  (Childress 
et  ai.  1984)  were  used  as  the  basis  of  a  family  of  curves 
that  predict  Pco,  from  pH  and  2CO2 .  However,  although 
the  coelomic  fluid  and  hemolymph  are  quite  similar  in 
ionic  composition,  the  hemolymph  often  has  much  higher 
hemoglobin  content.  Because  hemoglobin  is  the  only 
protein  in  any  concentration  in  the  hemolymph  (Arp  et 
ai,  1987),  we  used  the  concentration  of  heme  as  an  in- 
dicator of  protein  content  in  these  fluids.  An  approximate 
correction  factor  for  hemoglobin  concentration  was  de- 
veloped by  equilibrating  subsamples,  brought  to  different 
concentrations  in  Riftia  saline,  of  the  same  hemolymph 
sample  with  gases  of  known  Pco,  and  then  measuring  the 
SCO2  in  these  subsamples  using  the  gas  chromatographic 
method.  These  subsamples  (0.909  and  3.554  mM  heme) 
were  equilibrated  with  2.09  torr  Pco,  and  a  final  pH  of 
7.70  (Arp  el  ai,  1987).  These  measurements  indicated 
that  the  effect  of  heme  concentration  on  2CO2  in  this 
range  was  0.50  mmole  2CO2/mmole  heme.  This  was 
added  to  the  final  equation  used  to  calculate  PCo;  as  a 
factor  that  changed  the  slope  of  the  relationship  between 
Pco,  and  SCO-,  at  different  pH  values.  The  equation  was: 
PCO*  =  (4.199  -  0.537  pH)  +  SCO2[(eA(- 1.845  pH 
+  13.396)]  +  0.0667(heme  -  0.79).  PCO:  in  the  medium 
was  estimated  from  the  medium  pH  and  2CO2  with  the 
pKapp  estimated  from  the  equation  given  by  Heisler 
(1984),  and  «CO2  (0.06345)  at  8°C  (Skirrow.  1975). 

Statistical  methods 

Statistical  analyses  were  carried  out  using  Statview  SE+ 
and  SuperANOVA  (Abacus  Concepts)  and  Fastat  (Systat 
Inc.).  The  Kendall  rank  correlation  was  used  to  test  for  a 
relationship  between  two  parameters  without  any  as- 
sumptions about  the  form  or  linearity  of  the  relationship. 
Testing  for  differences  in  the  medians  in  paired  data  sets 
employed  the  Wilcoxon  signed  rank  test.  The  Mann- 
Whitney  U  test  was  used  to  test  for  differences  in  medians 
between  unpaired  datasets.  Simple  and  multiple  linear 
regressions  of  raw  and  in  transformed  data  were  used  to 
describe  the  relationships  between  parameters. 

Results 

H  'hole  animal  metabolism 

Due  to  a  variety  of  equipment  problems,  only  one  such 
experiment  was  successfully  conducted.  In  this  expert- 


AUTOTROPHIC  FUNCTION  IN  Rll-TIA 


139 


ment,  two  worms  (8.7  and  5.0  g)  were  run  in  their  natural 
tubes  in  one  chamber  for  68  h.  This  experiment  was 
started  at  13.5°C  without  sulfide.  After  6  h,  sulfide  was 
added  continuously  and  14  h  later  the  animals  showed 
net  2CO2  uptake  (autotrophy).  For  the  next  20  h  the  ef- 
fects of  different  sulfide  concentrations  on  the  fluxes  of 
O2,  2H2S  and  2CO2  were  measured  while  maintaining 
O2  between  105  and  209  ^M  (Fig.  IB).  After  that  time 
the  temperature  of  the  system  was  lowered  to  8.4°C  over 
2  h,  and  a  similar  set  of  measurements  repeated  at  O2 
concentrations  between  72  and  2 1 1  nM  during  the  next 
24  h  (Fig.  1A).  The  set  of  observations  at  8.4°C  started 
at  92  nAt  2H2S,  decreased  in  steps  to  0.0  nM  2H2S,  and 
then  was  then  raised  to  41-49  pM  2H:S  for  6  h.  As  can 
be  seen  in  Figure  1A,  the  lower  2H2S  concentrations  re- 
sulted in  less  uptake  of  2CO2,  and  without  added  sulfide 
the  2CO2  balance  was  fully  heterotrophic  (  +  3.05  /umoles 
2CO2  g~'h~').  For  the  first  three  hours  after  the  reintro- 
duction  of  sulfide,  this  balance  remained  heterotrophic 
(+1.93  Mmoles  2CO2  g~'h~'.  high  point  at  43  nM  2H2S 
in  Fig.  1A),  but  autotrophy  was  reached  in  the  next  3  h 
(-0.64Mmoles2CO2g  '  h~',  at  49  ^/2H2S  in  Fig.  1A). 
The  worms  were  then  removed  and  their  tubes  replaced 
in  the  vessels.  The  tubes  alone  did  not  show  significant 
2CO2  flux  (<0. 1  /umole  2CO2  g  '  worm  h" ')  in  the  pres- 
ence of  130  ju/U  2H2S.  When  the  worms  were  dissected 
after  the  experiment,  S°  was  visible  in  their  trophosomes. 

These  data  demonstrate  that  these  worms  were  depen- 
dent on  2H2S  levels  greater  than  about  50  nM  to  break 
even  on  carbon  flux  and  more  than  90  nM  was  required 
for  maximum  uptake  of  2CO2.  They  also  show  that  the 
lag-time  for  changes  in  2CO2  flux  when  2H2S  was  re- 
moved was  short,  suggesting  that  use  of  stored  S°  was  not 
quantitatively  very  important.  In  contrast,  when  sulfide 
was  introduced  after  an  absence,  the  lag  time  was  relatively 
long  (3- 14  h). 

Both  O2  and  2CO2  flux  were  significantly  dependent 
on  the  2H2S  flux  (Fig.  1C).  The  slope  of  the  line  relating 
2CO2  flux  to  2H2S  flux  was  0.92  ±0.18  (95%  C.  I.)  in- 
dicating that  0.92  mole  CO2  was  fixed  for  each  mole  H2S 
consumed.  The  slope  of  the  line  relating  O2  flux  to  2H2S 
flux  (Fig.  1C)  was  1.14  ±  0.17  (95%  C.  1.),  indicating  that 
1.14  mole  O2  was  consumed  for  each  mole  2H2S  con- 
sumed. The  lines  relating  2CO:  and  O2  fluxes  to  2H2S 
flux  both  intercept  the  y-axis  at  virtually  the  fluxes  found 
for  the  worms  in  the  absence  of  sulfide.  This  indicates 
that  sulfide  does  not  interact  with  the  metabolism  of  car- 
bon or  O2  by  the  animal  tissues.  The  R.  Q.  in  the  absence 
of  sulfide  is  0.83  suggesting  a  metabolism  based  on  a  mix- 
ture of  the  major  substrates. 

The  autotrophic  Riftia  experiment  described  above 
failed  to  show  net  uptake  of  N2,  supporting  other  negative 
data  that  this  species'  symbionts  do  not  fix  N2.  A  prelim- 
inary study  has  also  been  carried  out  on  ammonia  flux 


CD 
_<D 
O 

E 


x 

13 


CD 
_O 
O 

E 


+      ICC^  -        -    y  =  1  96  •  0  046x.  R=  0  83 

0  -i 

X      IhLS                  y  =  -0  48  -  0  040x.  R=  0.91 

-2  - 

X    .    x+                                                                            A 

X    '-.+"*•                                                                        o   ,-r 
X        x                                                                                 °A  ° 

-4  - 
-6  - 

-8  - 
-10    : 

•^•V*                 x 

•*V. 

2  - 

+      ICC^  -        -    y  =  1  42    0  027x,  R=  0.49 

0   -H 

+                     X      It-LS                   y  =  -0  76  -  0  035x.  R=  0  62 

-2  - 

X  +  ++x                                                   -            B 
+                                              13.5-C 

-4   - 
-6   - 
-8  - 

.1  n 

\-*x^  .x'                                           x 

X«         x 

>" 

.  .    • 

• 

50  100          150         200         250         300 


Figure  1 .  Riftia  pac/iypnla  metabolic  fluxes  in  a  flowing  water,  pres- 
sure respirometer  system.  Closed  circles  are  oxygen  fluxes,  x  symbols 
are  sulfide  fluxes,  and  crosses  are  CO;  fluxes.  (A)  Fluxes  presented  as 
functions  of  the  ambient  sulnde  concentrations  measured  at  8.4°C.  (B) 
Fluxes  presented  as  functions  of  the  ambient  sulnde  concentrations  mea- 
sured at  13.5°C.  (C)  Fluxes  at  both  temperatures  combined,  presented 
as  functions  of  sulnde  consumption  rate  as  manipulated  by  controlling 
the  sulfide  concentration  around  the  worms. 


in  Riftia.  Three  different  animals  (3. 3,  6.1,  and  17.2  g  wet 
weight)  in  flowing  water  pressure  respirometers  in  heter- 
otrophic carbon  balance  showed  appreciable  rates  of  am- 
monia excretion  (0.07,  0.19,  and  0.27  ^mol  g"1  h~',  re- 
spectively). 

Hemolymph  parameters  after  capture  and 
maintenance  without  sulfide 

In  these  experiments,  several  properties  related  to  au- 
totrophic metabolism  in  Riftia  pachyptila  were  followed 
over  time,  after  capture  and  recovery  of  the  tubeworms. 


140 


J.  J.  CH1LDRESS  /;/   AL 


Five  worms  were  sacrificed  immediately  after  capture,  and 
their  hemolymph  and  coelomic  fluid  pH,  2CO2,  2H2S, 
and  S2O32  as  well  as  trophosome  S°  concentrations  were 
measured.  Nine  other  R.  pachyptila  were  placed  in  high- 
pressure,  flowing-water  aquaria  immediately  after  recovery 
and  maintained,  under  pressure  (120  atm),  in  seawater 
without  added  sulfide  for  varying  periods  of  time  before 
sacrifice  and  analysis.  The  initial  values  found  (Table  I) 
were  comparable  to  those  found  previously  for  this  species 
(Childress  el  at..  1984)  with  2CO2  being  quite  elevated 
and  pH  values  being  quite  low.  This  indicated  that  the 
worms  were  probably  withdrawn  into  their  tubes  and  an- 
aerobic while  they  were  being  brought  to  the  surface.  Data 
following  recovery  in  the  aquarium  system  supports  the 
same  conclusion.  Hemolymph  and  coelomic  pH  rose  and 
2CO2  concentration  declined  (from  very  high  levels  found 
immediately  after  capture)  after  the  animals  were  main- 
tained for  one  or  more  days  under  pressure  (Table  I). 
Hemolymph  2H2S  concentrations  in  the  freshly  collected 
animals  were  substantial,  ranging  up  to  1.75  mA/(mean 
of  0.71  mA/  Table  I),  decreased  rapidly  in  animals  main- 
tained under  pressure  in  the  absence  of  added  sulfide,  and 
was  undetectable  after  three  and  five  days  (Table  I).  Tro- 
phosome S°  declined  significantly  with  time  as  well,  ap- 
proaching zero  after  3  to  5  days  (Table  I).  Thiosulfate 
concentrations  in  the  hemolymph  of  R.  pachyptila  were 


always  very  low  (less  than  36  pM,  average  =  24  ^Af)  and 
did  not  decline  during  the  five  days  in  captivity  (Table  I). 
To  examine  the  hypothesis  that  the  pattern  of  high 
2CO:  and  low  pH  found  in  the  hemolymph  of  freshly 
recovered  worms  resulted  from  oxygen  deprivation,  we 
maintained  two  individuals  for  24  h  in  the  flowing  water 
aquarium  system  at  14  juA/  O2  and  15  nM  2H2S.  Prior 
to  this  experiment  these  worms  had  been  kept  in  the 
aquarium  system  for  2  days  with  no  sulfide  and  more 
than  100  nAIO2.  The  hemolymph  pH  was  depressed  (6.48 
and  6.82),  supporting  the  suggestion  that  depressed  pH 
values  after  recovery  are  the  result  of  anaerobic  metabo- 
lism (Childress  cl  a/..  1984).  The  2CO2  values  were  low 
(3.357  and  3.280  mAl),  but  at  the  low  pH  values  these 
represent  high  Pco,  values  (13.9  and  8.0  torr).  The  failure 
of  these  worms  to  accumulate  the  higher  2CO2  concen- 
trations found  in  freshly  recovered  worms  (Table  I)  prob- 
ably resulted  from  their  plumes  remaining  extended  and 
thus  continuing  to  exchange  CO2  with  the  medium  during 
the  experiment.  In  contrast,  during  recovery  from  the 
bottom,  worms  were  constrained  in  a  box  and  could  not 
extend  their  plumes  to  exchange  gases.  This  is  consistent 
with  observations  that  Riftia  pachyptila  individuals  release 
substantial  amounts  of  2CO2  to  the  medium  under  hyp- 
oxic  conditions  (Childress  et  a/.,  1984).  The  hemolymph 
2H2S  contents  were  substantial  (5.497  and  5.013  mAl) 


Table  I 

Riftia  pachyptila  hemolymph,  coelomic  fluid,  and  trophosome  parameters  immediately  after  capture  and  alter  maintenance 

in  the  ah\ence  <>/  sulfide  inflowing  water,  pressure  (120  atm)  aquaria 


Days  after 

SCO, 

2H:S 

s2o3:- 

S° 

capture 

n 

Tissue 

pH 

(m  moles/1) 

(mmoles/l) 

(  mmoles/l) 

(%wet  wt.) 

0 

5 

Hemolymph 

7.07  ±  0.07 

10.37  ±  1.05 

0.714  ±  0.332 

0.024  ±  0.004 

5 

Coelomic 

7.14  ±  0.78 

11.56  ±  2.81 

0.089  ±  0.087 

0.013  ±0.017 

5 

Trophosome 

2.76    ±  1.38 

1 

1 

Hemolymph 

7.39 

7.78 

0.066 

0.000 

1 

Coelomic 

7.48 

8.67 

0.000 

0.000 

1 

Trophosome 

1.94 

3 

2 

Hemolymph 

7.38,  7.47 

5.48.  9.45 

0.000.  0.000 

0.000.  0.0  1  3 

2 

Coelomic 

7.42,  7.39 

6.39.  9.17 

0.000.  0.000 

0.000.  0.000 

2 

Trophosome 

1.75,0.03 

5 

5 

Hemolymph 

7.49  ±  0.40 

5.91  ±0.25 

0.000  ±  0.000 

0.014  ±0.013 

5 

Coelomic 

7.59  ±0.12 

5.91  ±0.56 

0.000  ±  0.000 

0.003  ±  0.006 

5 

Trophosome 

0.092  ±0.21 

Test  of  change  over  time 

in  captivity  (Kendall 

rank  correlation 

tan.  P  =) 

Hemolymph 

0.67,0.0014 

-0.77,  0.0005 

-0.70,  0.0009 

-0.29.0.19 

Coelomic 

0.71,0.0007 

-0.77,  0.0005 

-0.55,  0.0085 

-0.20,  0.35 

Trophosome 

-0.54,  0.0097 

"n"  indicates  the  number  of  worms  and  samples  at  each  time  period  and  the  parameter  values  are  shown  as  mean  ±  standard  error  of  the  mean. 
-CO;  and  2H:S  indicate  the  total  concentration  ot  all  forms  of  these  substances,  released  by  acidification  of  the  samples  in  the  process  of  analysis. 
The  Kendall  rank  correlation  tests  the  significance  of  changes  over  time  in  captivity  (underlined  tail  values  indicate  P  <  0.05)  and  are  listed  beneath 
each  parameter  tested. 


AUTOTROPHIC  FUNCTION  IN  Rll-'TlA 


141 


Table  II 

Coelomic  /hud  hemoglobin  concentrations  as  /'unctions  of  heinolymph  hemoglobin  concent  rations  in  Riftia  pachyptila  alter  maintenance  (24  h)  in 
high-pressure  (120  aim),  flowing-water  aquaria  ill  various  fixed  ~2<H:S  concentrations  >0.0  and  <S(W  ^M. 
Data  on  freshly  collected  worms  from  Arp  et  al.  (1987) 

Wilcoxon  signed-rank 


[coelomic]  -  a  +  b[hemolymph] 

Coel:Hemo 

+  ,  =,  - 

P 

Parameter 

n                   b  ±  95%  CI 

a                      r 

P 

All  Worms 
Heme  (mAf) 
Hemoglobin  FI  (mAf) 
Hemoglobin  FI1  (m.l/) 
Data  from  Arp  et  al  (1987) 
Hemoglobin  FI  (mAf) 
Hemoglobin  FI1  (m.l/) 

34                0.304  ±  0.204 

0.873                0.47 
0.04 
0.02 

0.05 
0.633                0.39 

0.005 
0.145 
0.187 

0.70 
0.0059 

1.0.  33 
0,  0,  34 

<0.0001 
<0.0001 
0.228 

0.0002 
0.013 

34 
34 

18 
18                0.413  ±0.276 

18,0.  16 
0.0.  18 

11.0.  7 

"n"  indicates  the  number  of  worms  and  samples  and  the  regression  coefficients  are  shown  ±95%  confidence  intervals  (CI ).  FI  is  the  large  hemoglobin 
aggregate  (1.700.000  Mr)  and  FII  is  the  smaller  aggregate  (400.000  Mr)  described  by  Terwilliger  et  al  (1980)  and  Arp  el  al.  (1987).  Underlined 
regression  coefficients  and  Wilcoxon  distributions  are  significant  at  the  level  of  at  least  P  <  0.05.  Not  all  analyses  were  completed  on  all  specimens. 
Regressions  are  given  only  when  they  are  significant  at  the  level  of  at  least  P  <  0.05. 


and  apparently  in  equilibrium  (0.52  and  0.57  fractional 
sulnde  saturation)  with  the  external  2H2S  (Fig.  5A,  B). 
This  is  consistent  with  uptake  being  due  solely  to  the 
binding  of  sulnde  by  the  hemoglobins. 

Functioning  of  titbeworms  exposed  to  various 
concentrations  of  oxygen  and  sulfide 

To  test  the  existing  hypotheses  concerning  carbon 
dioxide  and  sulnde  transport  in  the  hemolymph  (Rau  and 
Hedges,  1979;  Arp  and  Childress,  1983;  Childress  et  a/.. 
1984;  Arp  et  al..  1985;  Felbeck,  1985;  Fisher  et  al..  1989; 
Fisher  et  al..  1990)  and  to  examine  responses  of  this  species 
to  different  external  sulnde  concentrations,  a  series  of  ex- 
periments were  conducted  in  which  individual  tubeworms 
were  maintained  under  different  conditions  before  dis- 
section and  analysis.  In  these  experiments,  R.  pachyptila 
individuals  were  maintained  in  the  high-pressure  flowing- 
seawater  aquaria  in  the  absence  of  sulnde  for  two  days 
after  capture.  This  allowed  the  worms  to  recover  from 
capture  and  to  metabolize  most  of  their  internal  stores  of 
inorganic  sulfur  compounds.  These  worms  were  then  ex- 
posed continuously  to  constant  concentrations  of  sulfide 
(0-805  nAl  2H2S)  for  24  to  36  h  while  in  the  high-pressure 
( 120  atm)  flowing-seawater  aquaria  at  8°C.  Oxygen  con- 
centrations in  the  seawater  during  these  experiments  were 
between  0  and  276  \iM.  After  the  sulfide  exposure,  the 
worms  were  dissected  and  samples  taken.  The  hemolymph 
and  coelomic  fluid  samples  were  analyzed  for  pH,  2CO2, 
2H2S,  S2O3:  ,  and  the  two  hemoglobin  fractions.  Tro- 
phosome  samples  were  analyzed  for  S°.  Upon  dissection, 
seven  of  the  worms  were  found  to  have  substantial 
amounts  of  trophosome  that  appeared  to  be  in  poor  con- 


dition (see  Materials  and  Methods),  and  these  were 
dropped  from  further  consideration,  leaving  43  worms  in 
the  study.  Extensive  exploration  of  the  data  with  scatter- 
plots  suggested  that  the  data  could  best  be  presented  in 
two  groups;  one  of  these  consisted  of  28  animals  kept  at 
O2  concentrations  greater  than  42  juA/  and  whose  he- 
molymph pH  was  greater  than  7.2.  These  worms  showed 
evidence  of  autotrophy  and  blood  circulation  (to  be  dis- 
cussed below).  The  other  group  of  1 5  worms  consisted  of 
individuals  kept  at  O2  concentrations  of  42  \iM  or  less  (8 
worms)  and  individuals  whose  hemolymph  pH  was  less 
than  or  equal  to  7.2(11  worms).  The  low  O2  apparently 
limited  sulfide  oxidation  while  the  low  pH  values  appar- 
ently indicated  anaerobic  metabolism  in  the  worms  due 
to  behavioral  (remaining  contracted  in  the  tubes)  or  un- 
detected physiological  constraints.  In  the  figures  and  tables 
to  be  presented,  the  numbers  for  each  analysis  are  often 
less  than  the  total  number  of  individuals,  because  not  all 
analyses  were  successfully  executed  on  all  specimens. 

The  heme  contents  of  the  hemolymph  and  coelomic 
fluid  samples  of  the  worms  were  significantly  correlated, 
but  the  hemolymph  samples  had  much  higher  heme  con- 
centrations than  did  the  coelomic  fluid  ones  (Table  II). 
There  was  no  significant  correlation  between  the  concen- 
trations of  the  large  hemoglobin  (FI)  in  the  two  com- 
partments, but  the  concentration  in  the  hemolymph  was 
always  much  higher  (Table  II,  Fig.  2).  In  contrast,  there 
was  no  significant  difference  in  the  concentrations  of  the 
small  hemoglobin  (F  II)  between  the  two  compartments 
(Table  II).  However,  because  there  was  no  significant  cor- 
relation between  the  concentrations  in  the  two  compart- 
ments, it  is  apparent  that  they  are  not  confluent.  Arp  et 
al.  (1987)  suggested  that  these  two  compartments  may  be 


142 


J    J.  CHILDRESS  ET  AL 


Hemoglobin  Fraction  I  (mM) 

Figure  2.  Frequency  distributions  of  the  concentrations  of  the  large 
hemoglobin  [FI,  1.7  X  10"  Mr,  (Terwilliger  el  al.  1980;  Arp  el  at ..  1987)] 
in  coelomic  fluid  and  hemolymph  of  Ri/tia  pachvptila  kept  for  24  h  at 
different  external  sultide  and  d  concentrations. 


1.2  - 


O         °8  H 

'E 

°         0.4  H 
01 

o 


-    y  =  00133*0  176x  .  R=  090 


4  6  8  10 

Hemolymph  XH2S  (mM) 


12 


Figure  3.  Coelomic  fluid  -H,S  as  a  function  of  hemolymph  iH:S 
in  Rifia  pachypltla  kept  for  24  h  at  different  external  sulfide  concentrations 
and>42 


confluent  at  the  size  of  the  smaller  hemoglobin,  because 
the  concentrations  in  the  two  compartments  were  signif- 
icantly correlated.  However,  re-analysis  showed  that  while 
the  concentrations  were  significantly  correlated  in  their 
data,  the  coelomic  fluid  had  higher  concentrations  pre- 
cluding confluence  for  this  size  molecule  (Table  II). 

Although  the  two  compartments  do  not  exchange  he- 
moglobin molecules,  it  is  apparent  that  small  dissolved 
molecules  are  readily  exchanged,  because  the  higher  O2, 
higher  pH  group  had  highly  significant  correlations  be- 
tween the  values  of  all  of  the  measured  parameters  be- 
tween the  two  compartments  (Table  III).  In  addition,  there 
were  significant  differences  in  the  values  of  all  of  these 


parameters,  except  thiosulfate,  between  the  compart- 
ments, apparently  resulting  from  their  interactions  with 
the  hemoglobins.  2H2S  was  in  much  higher  concentra- 
tions in  the  hemolymph  (Fig.  3)  than  in  the  coelomic 
fluid  because  of  the  much  higher  concentrations  of  he- 
moglobin FI,  which  binds  3  moles  of  sulfide  per  mole  of 
heme,  although  this  binding  is  not  to  the  heme  group 
itself  (Arp  el  al.,  1987),  versus  1  mole  of  sulfide  per  mole 
of  heme  for  F  II  (Fig.  2).  These  correlations  and  distri- 
butions indicate  that  these  worms  were  circulating  their 
blood  effectively.  In  contrast,  the  low  O2,  low  pH  group 
had  no  significant  correlations  between  the  two  com- 
partments for  these  parameters,  and  the  values  were  sig- 


Table  III 

Riftia  pachyptila  coelomic  fluid  parameters  as  a  function  of  the  same  parameters  in  liemolymph  after  maintmancc  (24  h)  of  the  worms  in  flowing 
water,  pressure  (120  atml  aquaria  at  various  fixed  ^.H;S  concentrations  between  0.0  and  600  ^M 
with  external  O2  concentrations  >  42  ^M  ami  hemolymph  pH  >  7.2 


Wilcoxon  signed-rank 

[coelomicj  -  a  +  blhemolymphj 

Coel:Hemo 

Parameter 

n 

b  ±  95%  CI 

a 

r 

P 

+,  =,  - 

P 

pH 

26 

0.841  ±0.394 

1.223 

0.45 

0.0002 

16.  2,  8 

0.042 

£CO2  (mM) 

27 

0.990  ±0.105 

0.520 

0.94 

<0.0001 

25,  0.  2 

<O.OOOI 

PCOJ  (torr) 

20 

0.744  ±0.171 

0.638 

0.82 

<0.0001 

6,  0,  14 

0.04 

2H2S(mA/) 

27 

0.  1  76  ±  0.034 

0.013 

0.81 

<O.OOOI 

0,  7,  20 

<O.OOOI 

°>c-  Hb  sulfide  saturation 

20 

0.759  ±  0.123 

-0.019 

0.90 

<0.0001 

0,  7,  13 

0.0015 

Free  2H,S  (mA/) 

19 

0.1  40  ±0.031 

0.002 

0.84 

<0.0001 

0.6,  13 

0.0015 

FreeH2S(mA/) 

19 

0.  1  33  ±  0.024 

0.0003 

0.89 

<0.0001 

1,6,  12 

0.0024 

S2O32~  (mA/) 

22 

1.54    ±0.25 

-0.021 

0.89 

<0.0001 

7,3.  12 

0.41 

"n"  indicates  the  number  of  worms  and  samples  and  the  regression  coefficients  are  shown  ±95%  confidence  intervals  (CI).  -CO;  and  -HiS  indicate 
the  total  concentration  ot  all  forms  of  these  substances,  bound  and  free,  released  by  acidification  of  the  samples  in  the  process  of  the  analyses.  Free 
-H,S  is  an  estimate  of  the  free  sulfide  of  all  molecular  species.  Free  H2S  is  an  estimate  of  the  free  (i.e..  unbound)  concentration  of  this  molecular 
species.  Underlined  Wilcoxon  distributions  are  significant  at  the  level  of  at  least  P  <  0.05.  These  data  include  observations  for  all  parameters  for 
seven  animals  that  were  not  exposed  to  sulfide  during  the  experiment  and  had  internal  sulfide  concentrations  of  zero.  Not  all  analyses  were  completed 
on  all  individuals  studied. 


AUTOTROPH1C  FUNCTION   IN  R1FTIA 


143 


nificantly  different  for  only  three  of  the  parameters  (Table 
IV).  Such  lack  of  equilibration  indicates  a  lack  of  oppor- 
tunity for  exchange  between  the  two  fluids,  suggesting 
that  circulation  was  impaired  in  this  group  of  worms. 

Because  the  hemolymph  and  coelomic  fluid  parameters 
were  always  parallel  and  closely  correlated,  and  because 
the  low  O2 ,  low  pH  worms  do  not,  for  the  most  part,  show 
signs  of  autotrophy  and  effective  circulation,  the  hemo- 
lymph parameters  from  the  higher  O:,  higher  pH  worms 
will  be  emphasized  in  considering  the  responses  of  the 
internal  parameters  to  external  sulfide  (Table  V).  The  low 
O2,  low  pH  group  (Table  VI)  will  be  considered  primarily 
in  contrast  to  the  other  group. 

In  the  higher  O2  group,  hemolymph  and  coelomic  2H2S 
were  correlated  with  external  2H2S.  They  were  at  least 
one  order  of  magnitude  higher  than  the  external  concen- 
tration in  all  cases  (Table  V,  VII,  Fig.  4A),  clearly  dem- 
onstrating the  ability  of  this  worm  to  concentrate  sulfide 
from  its  environment.  However,  their  hemoglobin  was 
maintained  well  below  sulfide  saturation  at  all  external 
sulfide  concentrations  tested  (Fig.  5A)  showing  50%  sat- 
uration at  an  external  2H2S  of  122  ^M  as  compared  to 
an  /'/;  vitro  affinity  of  50%  saturation  at  1 1.2  fj.Af  2H:S 
(Fisher  el  <//..  1988a).  The  hemolymph  free  2H2S  and  free 
H2S  also  increased  with  external  2H2S,  but  remained 
about  an  order  of  magnitude  lower  than  the  external  con- 
centrations (Table  V,  Fig.  5B,  C).  Thus,  although  the  2H:S 
concentration  in  the  hemolymph  was  much  higher  than 
outside  the  worm,  there  was  a  significant  gradient  from 
the  outside  to  the  inside  for  the  free  chemical  species.  The 
latter  gradient  could  only  be  maintained  by  the  con- 
sumption of  sulfide  within  the  worm,  presumably  by  the 
symbionts. 


In  contrast,  the  low  O2.  low  pH  group  shows  only  three 
barely  significant  correlations  between  external  SH2S  and 
any  of  the  hemolymph  sulfide  parameters  (Table  VI). 
Further,  the  hemolymph  sulfide  saturation  is  close  to  that 
expected  in  vitro,  and  50%.  saturation  is  close  to  the  //; 
vitro  value  (3.3  and  1 1 .2  pM  2H2S,  respectively,  Fig.  5A). 
Thus,  the  ability  of  these  worms  to  concentrate  2H2S  in 
their  hemolymph  appears  to  be  explained  entirely  by  the 
binding  of  sulfide  by  the  hemoglobins.  In  addition,  the 
hemolymph  free  2H2S  and  free  H2S  were  essentially  in 
equilibrium  distributions  with  the  corresponding  external 
parameters  (Fig.  5B,  C)  indicating  that  no  gradient  for 
passive  uptake  exists  in  these  worms.  Symbiont  sulfide 
oxidation  had  apparently  essentially  ceased  under  these 
conditions  so  the  hemoglobins  could  no  longer  function 
to  depress  free  sulfide  concentrations. 

It  is  also  apparent  from  the  data  that  sulfide  is  the  only 
sulfur  compound  of  importance  in  the  hemolymph.  Al- 
though thiosulfate  was  found  in  both  groups,  and  increases 
significantly  in  the  presence  of  external  sulfide  (Table  V), 
it  is  typically  less  than  hemolymph  2H2S  by  more  than 
one  order  of  magnitude  (Fig.  4). 

The  hemolymph  2CO2  and  Pco,  values  are  both  re- 
duced at  higher  external  2H:S  concentrations  in  the 
higher  O2  group  (Table  V,  VII,  Fig.  6A,  B),  suggesting  the 
removal  of  inorganic  carbon  by  the  autotrophic  sym- 
bionts. There  were  no  significant  relations  between  these 
parameters  for  the  low  O2,  low  pH  group  (Table  V,  Fig. 
6A.  B).  In  addition,  the  internal  PCo,  was  higher  than  the 
external  under  virtually  all  conditions  (Fig.  6B,  Table  V, 
VI)  precluding  uptake  by  passive  diffusion  into  the  he- 
molymph. 


Table  IV 

Coelomic  fluid  parameters  ax  a  Junction  oj  the  same  parameters  in  hemolymph  oj  Riftia  pachyptila  after  maintenance  {24  h)  in  high-pressure  (120 
aim),  flowing-water  aquaria  at  various  fixed  sulfide  concentralicinx  between  0  13  and  SOI)  ^M 
with  O:  concentrations  <  42  /iM  or  hemolymph  pH  <  7.2 


[coelomic]  =  a  +  b[hemolymph] 


Parameter 


b  ±  95%  CI 


Wilcoxon  signed-rank 


Coel:Vasc 


pH 

15 

0.19 

0.105 

11.0.4 

0.125 

SCO;  (mM) 

14 

0.45 

0.054 

10.0,3 

0.022 

PCOl  (torr) 

13 

0.05 

0.47 

0.  0,  1  3 

0.0015 

2H2S(mAf) 

14 

0.00 

0.96 

2,0,  12 

0.0019 

%  Hb  sulfide  saturation 

13 

0.03 

0.56 

5.0.  8 

0.34 

Free2H,S(m.U) 

13 

0.04 

0.49 

5.0,  8 

0.60 

Free[H2S](mM) 

13 

0.03 

0.55 

4.0.9 

0.34 

S2O32~  (mM) 

7 

0.04 

0.691 

5,0,2 

0.13 

"n"  indicates  the  number  of  worms  and  samples  and  the  regression  coefficients  are  shown  ±95%  confidence  intervals  (CI).  2CO2  and  SH:S  indicate 
the  total  concentration  of  all  forms  of  these  substances,  released  by  acidification  of  the  samples  in  the  process  ol  the  analyses.  Free  2H2S  is  an  estimate 
of  the  unbound  sulfide  of  all  molecular  species.  Free  [H2S]  is  an  estimate  of  the  concentration  of  this  molecular  species.  Underlined  Wilcoxon 
distributions  are  significant  at  the  P  <  0.05  level.  No  regressions  were  significant  at  the  P  <  0.05  level  and  therefore  none  are  listed. 


144 


J.  J.  CHILDRESS  ET  Al. 
Table  V 


Riftia  pachyptila  liemolvmph  parameters  and  S°  in  trophosome  as  Junctions  of  external  conditions  after  maintenance  (24  h)  in  high-pressure  1 1 2/1 
aim),  flowing-water  aquaria  in  various  fixed  2//,5  concentrations  greater  than  0.0  and  less  than  600  /jM  (external  O:  concentrations  >  42  ^M  and 
hcniolvmph  pH  >  7.2).  kcndall  correlations,  but  not  the  regressions  or  Wilcoxon  tests,  include  seven  individuals  at  0.0  S//i 


X  Vanable 

Kendall  correlation 

[hemolymph  parameter]  =  aXb 

Wilcoxon  signed-rank 

External:Hemo 
+,  =,  -                    P 

Hemolymph 
Parameter 

n 

lau 

P  = 

n           b  ±  95%  CI                a 

r 

P 

X  =  External  ZH2S 

pH 

26 

-0.165 

0.24 

19 

0.05 

0.38 

SCO2  (mA/) 

27 

-0.629 

<O.OOOI 

20        -0.224  ±0.1  24           1.565 

0.44 

0.0014 

Pco,  (torr) 

26 

-0.529 

<0.0001 

19        -0.1  70  ±0.1  50          1.525 

0.25 

0.031 

2H-,S(mA/) 

27 

0.826 

<0.0001 

20          0.448  +  0.  PI        P.  53 

0.76 

<0.0001 

0.0,20              <0.0001 

Sulfide  saturation 

19 

0.772 

<o.ooo  i 

18           0.417  ±  0.138          1.057 

0.72 

<0.0001 

Free  2H,S  (mA/) 

IS 

0.861 

<0.000  1 

18           1.05    +0.41            0.105 

0.65 

<0.0001 

17,0.  1                 0.0002 

Free  H,S 

25 

0.832 

<0.0001 

18           1.086  ±0.438         0.0219 

0.63 

<0.0001 

S2032^  (mA/) 

22 

0.487 

0.0015 

15 

0.11 

0.13 

S°  (%  wet  wt.) 

24 

0.504 

0.0006 

14 

0.017 

0.66 

X  =  External  H^S 

Free  H2S 

25 

0.694 

<0.0001 

18           1053+0591          0.179 

0.47 

0.016 

17,0.  1                 0.0004 

A'  =  External  PCO! 

Pco,(mA/) 

25 

-0.361 

0.011 

18 

0.007 

0.74 

1.0,17                 0.0002 

Af  =  External  pi  I 

PH 

26 

0.123 

0.38 

19 

0.018 

0.68 

14,  OJ>                 0.0079 

"n"  indicates  the  number  of  worms  and  samples  and  the  regression  coefficients  are  shown  ±95%  confidence  intervals  (CI).  2CO2  and  -H2S  indicate 
the  total  concentration  of  all  forms  of  these  substances,  released  by  acidification  of  the  samples  in  the  process  of  the  analyses.  H,S  indicates  only  this 
chemical  species  itself.  "Free"  indicates  an  estimate  of  quantity  present  in  a  fluid  but  not  bound  to  the  hemoglobin.  Only  regressions  that  were 
significant  at  the  P  <  0.05  level  are  shown. 


Hemolymph  pH  appeared  to  be  unaffected  by  external 
or  internal  sulfide  or  external  pH  (Table  V,  VI,  VII,  Fig. 
6C),  although  it  is  somewhat  variable.  This  lack  of  inter- 
action suggests  that  pH  is  not  apt  to  be  a  significant  factor 
in  the  uptake  and  distribution  of  sulfide  or  inorganic  car- 
bon from  the  environment  into  the  hemolymph.  The 
strong  effect  of  low  O:  on  hemolymph  pH  is  probably 
due  to  the  accumulation  of  acidic  endproducts  of  anaer- 
obic metabolism. 

These  various  parameters  followed  the  same  trends  in 
the  coelomic  fluid.  However,  the  multiple  regression 
analyses  consistently  showed  that  the  strongest  predictor 
of  a  chemical  parameter  in  the  coelomic  fluid  is  not  an 
external  parameter  but  the  corresponding  parameter  in 
the  hemolymph.  This  indicates  that  the  route  of  transport 
to  the  coelomic  fluid  is  via  the  hemolymph  and  not  the 
body  wall. 


Discussion 


A  utotrophy 


Riftia  pachyptila,  like  all  vestimentiferan  tubeworms, 
lacks  a  mouth  and  a  gut  as  an  adult,  and  as  a  result,  its 
nutrition  has  been  a  matter  of  considerable  investigation 


and  speculation.  While  net  2CO2  uptake  (autotrophy)  by 
the  intact  symbioses  between  sulfur-oxidizing,  autotrophic 
bacteria  and  marine  invertebrates  living  around  deep-sea 
hydrothermal  vents  has  been  widely  assumed  (Cavanaugh 
el  a/..  1981;  Felbeck.  1981,  1985;  Felbeck  el  a/..  1981; 
Cavanaugh,  1985;  Southward,  1987),  the  data  presented 
here  are  the  first  actually  to  demonstrate  this  in  a  vesti- 
mentiferan tubeworm  or  any  hydrothermal  vent  animal. 
Actual  autotrophic  balance  for  the  tubeworm  symbiosis 
of  course  depends  on  the  reasonable  assumptions  that 
much  of  the  fixed  carbon  is  available  to  the  animal  tissues, 
and  the  production  of  mucus  and  loss  of  small  organic 
molecules  are  not  large  compared  to  the  rate  of  carbon 
fixation. 

The  only  previous  demonstration  of  autotrophic  bal- 
ance in  an  animal/bacterial  symbiosis  was  for  the  shallow- 
living,  gutless  protobranch  bivalve,  Solemya  reidi  (An- 
derson et  ai.  1987).  In  that  case,  the  clams  showed  a  max- 
imum net  2CO2  uptake  of  0.89  ^mole  g~'  h"1  which 
equals  about  0.24%  of  the  clam's  total  organic  carbon  per 
day.  In  contrast,  Riftia  pachyptila  apparently  has  a  con- 
siderably higher  maximum  rate  of  net  SCCK  uptake  (2.74 
/jmole  g  '  h  '  maximum  in  this  study).  This  high  rate, 
combined  with  the  relatively  low  carbon  content  of  5.5% 


AUTOTROPHIC  FUNCTION  IN  RII-T1A 
Table  VI 


145 


Hemolymph  parameters  anil  S°  in  trophosome  us  Junctions  oj  external  stil/ule  in  Riftia  pachyptila  alter  maintenance  (24  li)  in  high-pressure  (120 
aim).  Jhrning-water  aquaria  al  various  fixed  I//,5  concentrations  between  0.013  ant/  8I><>  ^M 
(external  O;  concentrations  <  42  ^M  or  hemolymph  pH  <  7.2) 

Wilcoxon  signed-rank 


Kendall  correlation 

[hemolymph  parameter]  -  aX 

External:Hemo 

+  ,     .  -                     P 

Hemolymph 
Parameter 

n 

tau 

P  = 

n 

b  ±  95%  CI                a                 r 

P 

,V  =  External  -//,S 

PH 

15 

-0.154 

0.42 

15 

0.00 

0.97 

2CO2(mA/) 

15 

-0.174 

0.37 

15 

0.10 

0.26 

PCO,  (torr) 

14 

-0.223 

0.91 

14 

0.02 

0.61 

2H,S(mA/l 

15 

0.385 

0.045 

15 

0.08 

0.30 

0,  0,  1  5               0.0007 

Sulfide  saturation 

14 

0.425 

0.034 

14 

0.098  ±  0.088         0.885         0.33 

0.033 

Free  SH,S(mA/) 

14 

0.438 

0.029 

14 

0.646  +  0.523          0.270          0.38 

0.020 

8,  0,  6                 0.55 

Free  H2S 

14 

0.402 

0.040 

14 

0.669  ±0.562         0.100         0.36 

0.023 

S2O,2~  (mA/) 

7 

-0.410 

0.20 

7 

0.56 

0.053 

S°  (%  wet  wt.) 

1  1 

-0.185 

0.94 

1  1 

0.028 

0.62 

A"  =  External  H,S 

Free  H,S 

14 

0.291 

0.148 

14 

0.20 

0.106 

4,0,  10                0.177 

.V  =  External  t>co. 

Pc02(niA/) 

14 

-0.205 

0.31 

14 

0.005 

0.82 

0,  0,  14                0.001 

X  =  External  />// 

PH 

15 

-0.216 

0.26 

15 

0.021 

0.87 

13,0,2               0.002 

"n"  indicates  the  number  of  worms  and  samples  and  the  regression  coefficients  are  shown  ±95'"  confidence  intervals  (CI).  2CO2  and  2H2S  indicate 
the  total  concentration  of  all  forms  of  these  substances,  released  by  acidification  of  the  samples  in  the  process  of  the  analyses.  H2S  indicates  only  this 
chemical  species  itself.  "Free"  indicates  an  estimate  of  quantity  present  in  a  fluid  hut  not  hound  to  the  hemoglobin. 


of  wet  weight  of  this  species  (Fisher  et  al.,  1988b),  results 
in  a  much  higher  estimate  of  1 .4%  of  the  tubeworm's  total 
organic  carbon  per  day.  This  suggests  a  high  potential 
growth  rate  in  this  species,  which  is  supported  by  some 
evidence  of  growth  in  length  in  situ  (Roux  et  al.,  1989). 
Very  rapid  growth  has  been  hypothesized  to  be  important 
in  this  species'  apparent  domination  of  young  vent  sites 
(Childress,  1988;  Fustec  el  al..  1988;  Hessler  et  al.,  1988). 
The  maximum  rate  of  carbon  fixation  (2.74  /umole 
2CO:  g  worrrr'tT1)  for  the  intact  symbiosis  corresponds 
to  a  rate  of  17.9  ^mole  2CO2  g  trophosome" 'h"1,  assum- 
ing that  trophosome  accounts  for  15.3%  of  the  wet  weight 
of  the  intact  symbiosis  (Childress  et  al.,  1984).  This  rate 
is  in  good  agreement  with  the  maximum  rates  of  fixation 
of  H14CO3  (13  to  28  Mmol  SCO2  g  trophosome'  V) 
observed  in  preparations  of  trophosome  tissue  from  Riftia 
pachyptila  containing  viable  endosymbiotic  bacteria  using 
sulfide  as  a  substrate  (Fisher  el  al.,  1989). 

Substrates  used  by  the  symbiosis 

Studies  of  the  isolated  symbionts  of  Riftia  pachyptila 
have  indicated  that  these  symbionts  use  only  sulfide  and 
not  thiosulfate  as  a  source  of  externally  derived  reducing 
power  (Belkin  et  al.,  1986;  Fisher  et  al.,  1989;  Wilmot 
and  Vetter,  1990).  The  data  presented  in  this  paper  sup- 


port the  view  that  vestimentiferan  tubeworms  are  spe- 
cialized to  supply  only  sulfide  to  their  symbionts  (Childress 
et  al..  1984),  because  sulfide  is  concentrated  from  the  me- 
dium and  is  quickly  used  by  the  symbionts  (Fig.  4,  5, 
Table  I).  In  contrast,  thiosulfate,  an  endproduct  of  animal 
sulfide  oxidation  (Vetter  et  al..  1987;  O'Brien  and  Vetter, 
1990),  is  always  at  a  very  low  concentration  in  the  he- 
molymph (Fig.  4)  and  is  not  quickly  used  by  the  symbiosis 
(Table  I).  The  lack  of  interaction  (either  inhibition  or  uti- 
lization as  substrate)  of  sulfide  with  the  animal  metabolism 
is  also  shown  by  the  fact  that  the  regressions  of  O-.  and 
2CO2  fluxes  versus  2H2S  flux  pass  essentially  through  the 
values  of  O2  and  CO2  flux  measured  in  the  absence  of 
SH2S  (Fig.  1C).  The  low  levels  of  sulfide  oxidase  activity 
reported  from  the  body  wall  of  R.  pachyptila  (Powell  and 
Somero,  1986)  are  apparently  of  little  significance  in  the 
overall  metabolism  of  this  species  because  so  little  thio- 
sulfate is  found  in  the  body  fluids.  Thus,  the  animal  me- 
tabolism has  little  interaction  with  sulfide,  delivering  it 
intact  to  the  symbionts. 

This  arrangement  is  quite  different  from  that  of  sym- 
biont-containing  bivalves,  which  appear  to  oxidize  sulfide 
to  thiosulfate  and  to  supply  this  to  the  symbionts.  Solernya 
reidi  mitochondria  can  produce  ATP  from  the  oxidation 
of  sulfide  to  thiosulfate  (Powell  and  Somero,  1985; 
O'Brien  and  Vetter,  1990),  which  can  then  be  supplied 


146 


J    J.  CHILDRESS  ET  AL 


E, 
CO 

C\J 

X 


o 

c\ 
CO 


0.2  - 


External  SH2S  (mM) 

Figure  4.  3H2S  and  thiosulfate  concentrations  in  hemolymph  and 
coelomic  fluid  in  Riflia  pachyptila  kept  for  24  h  at  different  external 
sulfide  concentrations  and  >42  jjA/CK.  (A)  The  broad  solid  line  represents 
equal  concentrations  of  2H2S  outside  the  worm  and  in  its  fluids.  The 
narrow  solid  line  and  the  closed  circles  apply  to  the  hemolymph  while 
the  dashed  line  and  crosses  apply  to  the  coelomic  fluid.  (B)  The  closed 
circles  represent  the  concentrations  of  thiosulfate  in  the  hemolymph  in 
these  same  worms. 

to  the  symbionts  to  support  their  metabolism  (Anderson 
et  at..  1987;  Vetter  el  ai.  1989).  In  this  species,  the  oxi- 
dation of  sulfide  apparently  has  substantial  effects  on  the 
animal  metabolism,  reducing  the  animal  carbon  oxida- 
tion, which  indicates  that  the  sulfide  oxidation  is  of  met- 
abolic significance  to  the  animal  tissues  (Anderson  et  al., 
1987).  Similarly,  the  symbionts  of  Balhymodiolus  ther- 
mophilus,  the  vent  mussel,  and  Calyplogena  magnified, 
the  vent  clam,  both  appear  to  be  able  to  use  thiosulfate 
to  drive  carbon-fixation  (Belkin  et  al.,  1986;  Childress  et 
al.,  1991),  and  the  animals  involved  accumulate  substan- 
tial thiosulfate  in  their  body  fluids  (Fisher  et  ai,  1988c; 
Childress  et  ai,  1991).  Whether  these  other  animals  can 
obtain  energy  from  the  oxidation  of  sulfide  remains  to  be 
tested. 

Rift i a  pachyptila  symbionts  are  dependent  on  the  im- 
mediate availability  of  sulfide  to  drive  significant  rates  of 
autotrophy.  While  the  symbionts  do  store  S°  at  concen- 
trations up  to  3200  /ig  atoms/g  fresh  weight  (Fisher  et  ai, 
1988b)  and  can  oxidize  the  stored  S°  in  the  absence  of 
external  2H2S  (Table  I),  the  metabolism  experiment  re- 
ported here  demonstrated  that  both  O2  and  autotrophic 
2CO2  fluxes  were  dependent  upon  an  external  supply  of 
2H2S.  The  stored  S°  did  not  support  a  detectable  rate  of 


2CO:  uptake  for  even  a  few  hours.  This  is  the  same  sit- 
uation found  in  the  bivalve  Solemya  reidi  (Anderson  et 
u/..  1987).  Thus,  it  appears  that  while  the  substantial  S° 
stores  often  found  associated  with  sulfur-oxidizing  sym- 
bionts (Vetter.  1985;  Somero  et  ai.  1989)  can  be  used  by 
the  symbionts,  these  rates  are  only  a  small  fraction  of  the 
rates  of  oxidation  of  sulfide  or  thiosulfate.  This  may  well 
be  the  case  for  free-living  sulfur-oxidizing  bacteria  as  well 
(Nelson  et  ai.  1986).  S°  sulfur  stores  may  be  of  signifi- 
cance for  the  survival  of  the  symbionts  during  times  of 
sulfide  deprivation,  but  apparently  do  not  represent  a  sig- 
nificant store  to  support  the  symbiosis. 

Respiratory  fluxes  in  response  to  sulfide 

Oxygen  and  2CO2  fluxes  in  Riflia  pachyptila  are  de- 
pendent upon  the  sulfide  flux.  Net  2CO2  uptake  requires 
the  presence  of  both  O2  and  sulfide.  About  90  ^M  2H2S 
was  necessary  to  reach  the  maximum  2CO2  uptake  rate 
(Fig.  1),  and  external  O2  concentrations  greater  than  42 
nM  appeared  to  be  necessary  for  the  use  of  sulfide  by  the 
symbionts  (Fig.  5).  These  concentrations  are  similar  to 
those  that  stimulate  maximal  autotrophy  in  5.  reidi  (An- 
derson el  ai,  1987).  However,  autotrophic  2CO2  uptake 
by  the  R.  pachyptila  symbionts  in  the  intact  symbiosis 
did  not  appear  to  be  inhibited  by  external  sulfide  concen- 
trations up  to  600  pM  (Fig.  5B),  unlike  that  of  the  sym- 
bionts of  S.  reidi,  which  are  inhibited  in  the  intact  sym- 
biosis at  external  2H2S  concentrations  of  about  250  nAI 
(Anderson  et  al.,  1987). 

These  environmental  requirements  of  R.  pachyptila 
appear  to  match  closely  the  environmental  conditions 
where  the  species  is  found.  Where  Riflia  is  in  abundance, 
the  flow  of  vent  water  is  high,  2H2S  can  approach  350 
fj.M  in  the  vent  water  and  O2  in  the  ambient  water  is 
around  110  ^M  (Fisher  et  ai.  1988b;  Johnson  et  al., 
1988b).  In  situ  measurements  of  sulfide  and  O2  distri- 
butions around  the  tubeworms  have  suggested  that  they 
take  up  sulfide  from  concentrations  above  about  60  nAf 
and  O2  from  concentrations  above  70  fiAf,  with  maximal 
uptake  rates  from  concentrations  around  100  ^M  in  both 
cases  (Johnson  et  ai.  1988b).  The  worms  gain  access  to 
both  substrates  at  high  concentrations  because  the  water 
around  them  is  not  well  mixed,  and  they  are  therefore 
exposed  to  conditions  that  fluctuate  between  vent  water 
( 1 5°C,  350  nM  2H2S,  0  pM O2)  and  ambient  water  (2°C, 
0  fiM  SH2S,  1 10  |«A/  O2)  on  time  scales  of  fractions  of  a 
second  and  longer  (Johnson  et  ai.  1988a).  This  species 
then  appears  to  be  specialized  for  high  rates  of  autotrophic 
function,  and,  as  a  result,  it  requires  the  high  concentration 
and  supply  of  sulfide  associated  with  rapid  venting.  These 
stringent  habitat  requirements  make  Rijtia  pachyptila 
vulnerable  to  either  natural  reduction  in  vent  flow  over 
time  or  diversion  of  vent  flow  by  mussels.  R.  pachyptila 


AUTOTROPHIC  FUNCTION  IN  Rll-11  A 


147 


§ 

s 

.c 


0) 

1 

03 


13 
(jy 

_fZ 
Q. 

E 

"o 

E 

0) 

I 


0.8  - 


(n      0.6  - 

~03  .O 

§2   0.4 

'tsa 

03   03 

.>-  rn    0.2  - 


CO 

c\j 


0.1    --. 


0.01 


0.001    - 


CO 


10'4 

0.1 

0.01    i 

0.001    - 


CD 

ul       10 


H 1 1     I    I    I   I 


K  External  water  • 


0.01 


i 1 1 — I — i — i    i    | 1 r~ 

0.1 

External  IH2S  (mM) 


Figure  5.  Rillui  pachyplila  hemolymph  sullide  parameters  as  a  function  ofexternal  £H:S  concentrations 
in  worms  kept  for  24  h  at  fixed  SH;S  concentrations.  Closed  circles  and  narrow  solid  lines  represent  values 
from  worms  that  were  kept  at  >42  nM  O2  and  had  hemolymph  pH  values  >  7.2.  Open  circles  and  dashed 
lines  represent  values  from  worms  that  were  kept  at  <42  pM  O2  or  had  hemolymph  pH  values  <7.2. 
Regression  equations  for  the  plotted  lines  may  be  found  in  Tables  V  and  VI.  (A)  The  fractional  saturation 
of  the  hemoglobins  in  vivo  with  sulfide  as  estimated  from  the  hemoglobin  concentrations,  hemolymph  3H2S 
and  in  vitro  sulfide  binding  properties.  The  broad  solid  line  is  the  saturation  versus  2H;S  relationship 
determined  in  vitro  (Fisher  el  a/..  1988a).  (B)  The  relationship  between  free  (unbound)  iH:S  and  external 
-H;S  in  hemolymph.  The  broad  solid  line  represents  equal  concentrations  in  the  hemolymph  and  outside. 
(C)  The  relationship  between  free  (unbound)  H:S  and  external  3H2S.  The  x  symbols  and  broad  solid  line 
represent  the  external  H:S  concentrations  in  these  same  experiments. 


numbers  might  therefore  decline  at  a  vent  site  long  before 
venting  ceased,  as  has  been  observed  at  the  Galapagos 
Rift  Rose  Garden  site  (Hessler  et  ai.  1988). 

Molar  ^CO::O::^H,S  ratios 

The  maximal  measured  uptake  rates  of  O2,  2H2S,  and 
2CO2  by  Riftia  pacliyptila  are  high  (Fig.  1 );  they  are  about 


twice  those  of  5.  reidi  for  O2  and  2H2S  and  three  times 
that  of  S.  reidi  for  2CO:  (Anderson  et  a!.,  1987).  The 
relationships  between  the  O2  and  2CO2  fluxes  and  the 
2H2S  flux  provide  quantitative  estimates  of  the  depen- 
dences of  the  former  fluxes  upon  the  latter  (Fig.  1C).  As 
noted  earlier,  these  relationships  suggest  that  there  is  little 
direct  interaction  of  the  animal  metabolism  and  the  SH2S 
flux,  and  thus  they  apparently  reflect  the  symbiont  me- 


148  J    J    CHILDRESS  ET  AL 

Table  VII 

(.'omi\in\on\  ol  hemolymph  and  coelomic  fluid  parameters  in  Riftia  pachyptila  alter  maintenance  (24  h)  in  high  pressure  (120  aim),  flowing-water 
tu/itiirui  cither  in  ihe  absence  or  presence  (0.016  to  0.593  mM.  mean  =  0.19  mM  ~H,S)  of  sulfide 


Hemolymph 


Coelomic  lluid 


Wilcoxon  Coel:Hemo 

+.  =,  - 


Parameter 

0  sulfide 

sulfide 

0  sulfide 

sulfide 

0  sulfide 

sulfide 

pH 
2CO2  (mM) 
PCO!  (torr) 
2H,S(m;U) 

7.47  (+0.32.7) 
5.76(+0.66.  7) 

7.40  (±0.26,  19) 
2.78  (±0.25.  20) 

7.54  (0.05.  7) 
6.44  (±0.049.  7) 

7.44  (0.03,  20) 
2.78(0.26,  21) 

5,0.  2 
6,  0,  1 

14,  0.  5 

19.0.   1 

5.81  (+0.78,7) 

3.16  (+0.28,  18) 

4.95  (+0.00.  7) 

2.92  (+0.00,  19) 

1,0,6 

1.0.  17 

0.0    (7) 

5.38  (+0.61.  20) 

0.0    (7) 

0.95  (±0.12,  21) 

0.  7,0 
0.  3.  4 

0.  0.  20 

0.01  (+0,01,  7) 

0.06  (+0.01.  15) 

0.00  (±0.00,  7) 

0.06  (±0.02,  17) 

7.0.  8 

Parameter  values  are  shown  as  means  with  the  standard  errors  of  the  means  and  the  number  of  observations  in  parentheses.  For  the  hemolymph 
and  coelomic  fluid  comparisons  with  and  without  external  sulfide.  the  data  sets  were  compared  using  the  Mann-Whitney  U  test.  Single  underlined 
pairs  of  means  are  from  groups  that  have  null  hypothesis  P  values  <  0.05.  Double  underlining  indicates  lvalues  <  0.005.  The  relative  concentrations 
of  each  substance  were  compared  between  the  coelomic  and  vascular  compartments  using  the  Wilcoxon  signed-rank  test.  Single  and  double  underlining 
have  the  same  meanings  for  this  test  as  for  the  Mann-Whitney. 


tabolism.  The  regression  analyses  of  the  metabolism  ex- 
periment (Fig.  1C)  indicate  that  these  symbionts  fix  0.92 
mole  2CO:  using  1.14  mole  O2  and  1  mole  2H2S.  Using 
the  calculation  methods  of  Kelly  (1982),  the  thermody- 
namic  efficiencies  implicit  in  these  ratios  can  be  deter- 
mined. Given  a  requirement  of  496  kJ  to  reduce  CO2  to 
hexose,  and  a  AG  =  — 716  kJ  for  the  oxidation  of  sulfide 
to  sulfate,  the  resulting  efficiency  is  63%.  The  molar  ratio 
observed  for  5.  reidi,  0.38  ZCO2:0.92  O2:l  2H2S,  gives 
an  efficiency  of  40%  if  one  assumes  that  the  bacteria  are 
using  thiosulfate  (AG  =  -936  kJ  for  two  S  atoms)  (An- 
derson el  a!..  1987).  In  contrast,  studies  of  the  thermo- 
dynamic  efficiencies  of  free-living  bacteria  have  been  done 
using  very  different  methods,  and  they  have  shown  lower 
efficiencies.  The  studies  of  free-living  bacteria  have  used 
the  ymax  or  "true  growth  yields"  to  estimate  fixation  in- 
dependent of  maintenance  metabolism  (Kelly,  1982). 
Thermothrix  thiopara  has  the  highest  ratios  yet  deter- 
mined for  aerobic  sulfur-oxidizers  (0.58  2CO2:1  thiosul- 
fate at  72°C),  corresponding  to  a  thermodynamic  effi- 
ciency of  29%  (Mason  et  al,  1987).  While  the  ratios  and 
efficiencies  for  the  5.  reidi  and  R.  pachyptila  symbionts 
seem  unusually  high,  this  may  be  a  result  of  the  symbiotic 
lifestyle. 

Measurements  of  the  ymax  in  free-living  bacteria  are 
generally  made  in  a  chemostat  that  maintains  constant, 
optimal  conditions  for  the  growth  of  the  bacteria.  The 
production  of  bacterial  biomass  is  then  measured.  This 
situation  is  very  different  from  that  in  a  symbiosis  in  that 
microbial  growth  involves  the  synthesis  of  a  variety  of 
complex  compounds,  not  primarily  the  production  of 
small  organic  molecules  as  is  typical  of  animal/algal  sym- 
bioses  and  is  probably  typical  of  most  animal/bacterial 


symbioses  as  well.  In  .S.  reidi.  the  symbionts  "leak"  newly 
fixed  carbon  within  seconds  and  are  apparently  held  at  a 
very  low  rate  of  growth  (Fisher  and  Childress,  1986).  While 
much  less  is  known  about  this  aspect  of  the  R  pachyptila 
symbiosis,  it  too  is  believed  to  operate  primarily  by  the 
"leakage"  of  small  organic  compounds  from  the  bacteria 
to  the  animal  with  the  bacteria  being  held  in  a  state  of 
slowed  reproduction  (Felbeck.  1985).  Under  the  condi- 
tions present  in  these  symbioses  (low  microbial  growth 
rates,  synthesis  of  small  organic  compounds,  and  mainte- 
nance in  an  environment  controlled  by  the  host)  it  may 
be  possible  for  bacteria  to  achieve  unusually  high  effi- 
ciencies for  CO2  fixation. 

The  internal  consistency  of  the  molar  ratios  for  Riftia 
pachyptila  can  also  be  evaluated  from  the  O2:2H2S  ratios. 
The  ratio  of  1.14:1  falls  well  short  of  the  expected  2:1  if 
all  of  the  sulfide  is  oxidized  to  sulfate  in  the  absence  of 
other  reductive  processes.  However,  because  carbon  fix- 
ation is  a  reductive  process,  the  reducing  equivalents  used 
in  carbon  fixation  must  also  be  taken  into  account.  Fol- 
lowing the  reasoning  of  Kelly  (1982),  each  CO2  fixed  to 
the  level  of  CH2O  via  the  Calvin-Benson  cycle  requires 
4e~  and  4H+.  For  our  ratio  of  0.92  2CO2:1  2H2S,  CO2 
fixation  requires  0.92  X  4(H)  =  3.68  of  the  8(H)  available 
from  complete  oxidation  of  sulfide.  Thus,  8  --  3.68 
=  4.32(H)  remain  for  the  reduction  of  O2,  and  the  pre- 
dicted O2  uptake  would  be  4.32/8  X  2  =  1.08  O2,  com- 
pared with  our  value  of  1.14.  This  agreement  supports 
the  validity  of  the  observed  ratios. 

In  contrast,  the  ratios  determined  for  S.  reidi  showed 
a  considerable  discrepancy  (0.92  O2 : 1 2H2S  observed  ver- 
sus 1.62:1  calculated  as  above)  with  insufficient  O2  con- 
sumption seemingly  to  account  for  the  observed  fixation 


AUTOTROPHIC  FUNCTION  IN  R11-T1A 


149 


§ 


_ 
OJ 

0) 

E 
co 

CO 
D_ 

JZ 
Q_ 

E 
^ 

o 
E 

0) 


O 
O 


c\j 
O 

o 


10   - 


1   - 


0.1 
7.6 


5  - 


74  - 


73  - 


72 


H 1 — I     I    I   I 


-t * ( — I     I    I 


001 


0  1 

External  XH0S  (mM) 


Figure  6.  Rillui  pacliypnla  hemolymph  parameters  as  a  function  of  external  -H;S  in  worms  kept  for 
24  h  at  a  given  2H2S.  Closed  circles  and  solid  lines  represent  values  from  worms  which  were  kept  at  >42 
nM  Oi  and  had  hemolymph  pH  values  >7.2.  Open  circles  represent  values  from  worms  which  were  kept 
at  <42  n\l  O2  or  had  hemolymph  pH  values  <7.2.  Regression  equations  for  the  plotted  lines  may  be  found 
in  Table  V.  Where  no  line  is  plotted,  the  relationship  was  not  significant  (Tables  V  and  VI).  (A)  Hemolymph 
SCO2  as  a  function  of  external  —HiS.  (B)  Pco,  as  a  function  of  external  iH:S  in  hemolymph  and  in  the 
external  water  (x  symbols).  (C)  Hemolymph  pH  as  a  function  ol  external  2H;S. 


of  carbon  and  oxidation  of  sulfide  (Anderson  el  at.,  1987). 
This  discrepancy  was  attributed  to  the  interactions  be- 
tween the  animal  sulfide  and  carbon  metabolism.  There- 
fore, the  agreement  observed  for  R.  pachyptila  may  be 
yet  another  indicator  of  the  degree  to  which  the  animal 
metabolism  is  isolated  from  the  sulfur  metabolism  of  the 
symbiosis. 

Uptake  and  transport  processes 

The  data  presented  here  provide  much  new  information 
on  the  processes  for  the  uptake  and  transport  of  sulfide 
and  carbon  dioxide  that  are  operative  in  these  worms. 


The  central  role  of  the  sulfide-binding  hemoglobins  in 
sulfide  uptake,  transport,  and  toxicity  control  (Arp  and 
Childress,  1983;  Powell  and  Somero,  1983;  Childress  et 
ai,  1984;  Powell  and  Somero,  1986;  Fisher  et  a/..  1988a, 
1989)  is  fully  supported  by  these  data.  In  particular,  the 
worms  can  bind  sulfide  reversibly  in  vivo  (Table  I);  they 
can  concentrate  2H2S  from  the  medium  by  a  factor  of  1 
to  2  orders  of  magnitude  (Fig.  4A);  and  when  the  worms 
are  not  in  autotrophic  balance  (low  O2,  low  pH  group), 
the  hemolymph  hemoglobins  approach  equilibrium  sul- 
fide binding  (half  saturation  near  1 1  ^M  2H2S)  at  all  sul- 
fide concentrations  tested  (Fig.  5A).  In  addition,  free  2H2S 
and  free  H2S  in  the  hemolymph  of  these  worms  is  in  equi- 


150 


J.  J.  CHILDRESS  1:1    II 


librium  with  the  same  parameters  in  the  external  medium 
(Fig.  5B,  C).  Because  the  endpoint,  in  the  absence  of  au- 
totrophy,  appears  to  be  sulfide  equilibrium  between  the 
hemolymph  and  the  medium,  no  mechanism  other  than 
diffusion  appears  to  be  functioning  to  bring  sulfide  into 
the  hemolymph. 

When  the  worms  are  in  autotrophic  balance,  the  sym- 
bionts  remove  sulfide  from  the  hemolymph  at  a  sufficient 
rate  to  keep  the  hemoglobins  below  equilibrium  binding 
of  sulfide,  resulting  in  an  apparent  in  vivo  affinity  of  122 
nM  ZH2S  for  half  saturation  of  the  hemoglobins  (Fig. 
5A).  This  uptake  by  the  symbionts  is  sufficient  to  maintain 
the  hemolymph  free  2H2S  and  free  H2S  about  an  order 
of  magnitude  below  the  values  of  those  parameters  in  the 
external  medium  (Fig.  5B,  C).  This  provides  a  gradient 
to  drive  the  diffusion  of  sulfide  into  the  hemolymph.  The 
available  evidence  indicates  that  this  gradient  is  sufficient 
for  the  uptake  of  sulfide  by  the  worms.  Once  the  sulfide 
is  transported  to  the  trophosome,  it  presumably  diffuses 
from  the  hemolymph  into  the  bacteriocytes  and  subse- 
quently to  the  bacteria.  A  sulfide  binding  factor  found  in 
the  trophosome  may  also  be  important  in  this  process 
(Childress  et  a/..  1984). 

The  cooperative  role  of  the  hemoglobin  sulnde-binding 
and  the  symbiont  sulfide  consumption  in  controlling  he- 
molymph free  2H2S  concentrations  to  prevent  sulfide 
toxicity  to  either  the  host  tissues  or  the  symbionts  can  be 
appreciated  from  Figure  4  and  Figure  5.  For  example,  at 
an  external  2H2S  of  100  fiM,  the  internal  2H2S  is  4.5 
mM,  however,  the  hemoglobins  are  only  47%  saturated, 
and,  as  a  result,  the  free  2H2S  is  only  9.3  nAI  and  the  free 
H2S  is  1 .8  nM.  Above  300  /uA/ external  SH2S,  the  internal 
free  sulfide  rises  rapidly  due  to  the  increasing  saturation 
of  the  hemoglobins.  Because  50%  inhibition  of  R.  pa- 
chyptila  cytochrome  c  oxidase  activity  occurs  at  about  25 
nM  2H2S  in  vitro  at  pH  7.0  (Powell  and  Somero,  1986), 
the  observed  hemolymph  free  2H2S  concentrations  in- 
dicate a  significant  degree  of  protection  for  this  critical 
enzyme  at  the  usual  external  2H2S  concentrations  found 
in  this  species'  environment,  with  50%  inhibition  being 
reached  at  about  250  \i.M  external  2H2S  concentration 
(Fig.  5B).  While  the  sensitivity  of  the  R.  pachyptila  sym- 
bionts to  sulfide  in  vitro  has  not  been  precisely  defined, 
the  onset  of  inhibition  of  carbon  fixation  at  pH  7.5  appears 
to  occur  at  about  300  \iM  free  2H2S  (Fisher  el  al.  1989). 
In  autotrophic  worms,  such  concentrations  would  not  be 
reached  until  the  hemoglobin  was  more  than  90%  satu- 
rated, which  would  not  be  expected  until  external  wH2S 
concentrations  reached  more  than  900  ^Af  2H2S  (Fig.  5). 
Thus,  the  proposed  protective  role  of  the  hemoglobin  sul- 
fide binding  activity,  both  for  the  tubeworm  tissues  and 
the  symbionts,  is  supported  by  these  observations  of  in 
vivo  sulfide  concentrations. 


The  uptake  and  transport  of  inorganic  carbon  appear 
to  be  very  different  from  those  for  sulfide.  The  environ- 
mental pH  around  the  worms  ranges  from  7.0  in  vent 
water  to  7.9  in  ambient  water,  and  the  2CO2  is  about  the 
same  in  both  (K.  Johnson,  pers.  comm.).  While  O2  must 
be  taken  up  primarily  from  ambient  water  and  2H2S  from 
vent  water,  inorganic  carbon  could  be  taken  up  from  either 
or  both,  although  the  Pco,  values  would  be  higher  in  the 
vent  water.  The  hemolymph  pH  of  the  worms  appears  to 
be  somewhat  variable,  with  typical  values  being  between 
7.4  and  7.55.  The  hemolymph  pH  does  not  appear  to  be 
affected  even  by  the  large  2H2S  concentrations  that  it 
carries  at  high  external  sulfide  concentrations.  This  is  very 
different  from  the  situation  described  for  nonsymbiotic 
organisms  where  H2S  diffuses  into  the  organism  and  then 
dissociates  causing  a  drop  in  pH  (Jaques,  1936;  Groenen- 
daal,  198 1 ).  It  suggests  that  either  the  uptake  mechanism 
is  different  in  Riftiu  or  the  binding  mechanism  does  not 
result  in  the  release  of  H*  from  H2S.  Hemolymph  pH  is 
apparently  also  not  affected  by  the  2CO2  concentration 
variations  or  the  FT  produced  by  sulfide  oxidation.  He- 
molymph 2CO2  and  Pco,  values  in  R.  pachyptila  were 
unusually  high  for  a  worm  under  oxic  conditions  (pH 
=  7.47,  2CO2  =  5.76  mM,  Pco,  =  5.81  torr  at  8°C  in  the 
absence  of  sulfide  and  7.4,  2.78,  and  3.16,  respectively, 
in  the  presence  of  sulfide.  Table  VII).  For  comparison, 
Arenicola  marina  has  a  hemolymph  pH  of  about  7.53,  a 
2CO2  of  about  2.5  mM.  and  a  PCo:  of  about  1.1  torr  at 
7.5°C  (Toulmond,  1977).  Thus,  it  appears  that,  in  spite 
of  its  apparently  large  respiratory  surface  and  effective 
circulation  (Arp  et  al.,  1985;  Jones,  1981),  R  pachyptila 
has  unusually  high  internal  2CO2  and  PCo:  levels.  When 
the  worms  were  in  apparent  autotrophic  balance,  the  he- 
molymph SCO2  and  PCo,  were  decreased  significantly  as 
a  function  of  external  2H2S,  with  minimal  values  ap- 
proaching 1  mM  and  1  torr,  respectively,  but  were  still 
above  the  environmental  values  (Table  VII,  Fig.  6A,  B). 
While  we  believe  that  the  observed  distributions  represent 
conditions  in  the  worms  under  net  autotrophy,  it  is  pos- 
sible that  the  observed  distribution  could  result  from  the 
experimental  worms  not  being  in  net  autotrophic  balance. 
The  decrease  in  2CO2  under  autotrophic  conditions  is 
most  likely  due  to  the  demand  of  the  symbionts,  because 
they  have  been  shown  to  readily  fix  inorganic  carbon 
(Belkin  et  al..  1986;  Fisher  et  al..  1989).  The  implication 
of  the  observed  distributions  is  that  these  tubeworms  con- 
centrate 2CO2  to  relatively  high  PCo,  values  in  the  he- 
molymph and  then  depend  on  diffusion  through  the  bac- 
teriocytes to  supply  the  symbionts. 

This  hypothesis  is  supported  by  the  unusual  6L1C  values 
of  Riftia  pachyptila  of  between  -9  and  -15.6%o  (Rau, 
1981;  Fisher?/  al..  1988b,  1 990).  These  workers  have  sug- 
gested that  these  low  values  of  isotope  discrimination  re- 
sult from  carbon  fixation  in  this  species  operating  under 


AUTOTROPHIC  FUNCTION  IN  RIFTI.l 


151 


conditions  approaching  carbon  limitation,  as  can  happen 
in  marine  plankton  (Degens  el  a/..  1968).  Because  the  Km 
for  the  carbon  fixation  by  the  Riftia  symbionts  is  between 
400  and  700  ^M  2CO2  at  pH  7.5  (Fisher  el  ai.  1988d), 
the  hemolymph  carbon  dioxide  values  (as  low  as  1100 
tiM  SCO;  in  the  presence  of  sulfide)  might  well  be  low 
enough  to  limit  carbon  isotope  discrimination  under  con- 
ditions of  active  autotrophy. 

Relationship  between  coelomic  fluid  and  hemolymph 

The  data  presented  here  support  the  view  that  there  is 
free  exchange  of  small  molecules  between  the  coelomic 
fluid  and  hemolymph  (Childress  el  ai,  1984),  although 
this  exchange  does  not  extend  to  molecules  as  large  as  the 
hemoglobins.  The  much  higher  hemoglobin  concentration 
in  the  hemolymph  is  clearly  responsible  for  the  much 
higher  2H2S  in  that  fluid,  and  may  well  be  responsible 
for  differences  in  pH,  2CO2,  and  Pco,  as  well.  However, 
the  cause  of  the  consistently  lower  percent  sulfide  satu- 
ration, free  2H2S,  and  free  H2S  in  the  coelomic  fluid  is 
not  apparent  at  this  time.  The  new  data  reported  here 
support  the  concept  that  the  coelomic  fluid  is  a  reservoir 
of  O2,  2CO2,  and  2H2S  which  the  worms  can  use  to 
buffer  the  effects  of  brief  fluctuations  in  vent  flow  (Arp 
and  Childress,  1981;  Childress  et  ai.  1984). 

Model  of  the  functioning  of  the  intact  symbiosis 

Rift  in  puc/iyplila  appears  to  have  the  greatest  auto- 
trophic  potential  and  as  a  result  the  fastest  growth  rate  of 
any  of  the  sulfur-oxidizing  symbioses  investigated  to  date. 
It,  and  probably  all  vestimentiferans,  appears  to  be  unique 
among  the  studied  species  in  that  the  animal  is  specialized 
to  minimize  the  interaction  of  the  animal  metabolism 
with  sulfide  and  to  provide  only  sulfide  to  symbionts  that 
are  only  capable  of  using  sulfide.  Central  to  the  ability  of 
the  vestimentiferan  symbioses  to  use  sulfide  are  the  he- 
moglobins, which  reversibly  bind  both  sulfide  and  oxygen 
to  different  sites  simultaneously.  These  hemoglobins  en- 
able the  worms  to  concentrate  sulfide  from  the  medium 
and  by  almost  two  orders  of  magnitude.  Yet,  because  of 
the  high  affinity  of  the  hemoglobins  for  sulfide  as  well  as 
the  consumption  of  sulfide  by  the  symbionts,  which  holds 
the  hemoglobins  well  below  sulfide  saturation,  the  worms 
can  maintain  their  hemolymph  free  2H2S  concentrations 
an  order  of  magnitude  lower  than  external  2H2S  concen- 
trations. The  high  capacitance  of  the  hemolymph  for  sul- 
fide is  essential  for  the  transport  of  sufficient  quantities 
of  sulfide  to  the  symbionts  via  the  circulatory  system.  The 
low  free  sulfide  concentrations  are  essential  for  preventing 
the  inhibition  of  animal  metabolism  or  symbiont  carbon 
fixation  by  sulfide.  Diffusion  of  sulfide  across  the  plume 
into  the  hemolymph  appears  sufficient  to  explain  the 
movement  of  sulfide  into  the  worms.  Because  the  sym- 


bionts can  take  sulfide  from  the  hemoglobins,  diffusion 
from  the  hemolymph  into  the  bacteriocytes  in  the  highly 
vascularized  trophosome  may  well  be  sufficient  to  supply 
the  needs  of  the  symbionts. 

The  uptake  and  supply  of  O2  to  both  the  symbionts 
and  the  worm  tissues  is  apparently  accounted  for  by  the 
high  affinity  of  the  hemoglobins  for  oxygen  and  the  ability 
of  the  symbionts  and  the  tissues  to  use  O2  at  low  P02 
values.  The  combination  of  the  high  O2  affinity  and  the 
high  sulfide  affinity  is  responsible  for  the  ability  of  these 
hemoglobins  to  suppress  the  spontaneous  oxidation  of 
sulfide  by  O:  (Fisher  and  Childress.  1984). 

About  half  the  inorganic  carbon  fixed  by  the  symbionts 
is  potentially  derived  from  the  heterotrophic  metabolism 
of  the  symbiosis,  while  the  remaining  half  requires  the 
uptake  of  inorganic  carbon  from  the  medium.  The  he- 
molymph 2CO2  and  Pco,  are  apparently  elevated  above 
the  medium  by  some  mechanism,  other  than  a  pH-based 
one,  which  concentrates  carbon  dioxide  in  the  hemo- 
lymph. This  elevated  hemolymph  inorganic  carbon  can 
then  diffuse  into  the  bacteriocytes  and  to  the  bacteria, 
although  the  available  evidence  indicates  that,  at  maximal 
rates  of  autotrophy,  this  supply  may  approach  values  lim- 
iting the  rate  of  carbon  fixation.  The  supply  of  fixed  carbon 
from  the  symbionts  to  the  host  is  presumably  predomi- 
nantly via  small  organic  molecules  transported  in  the  he- 
molymph. 

Using  the  available  data,  one  can  evaluate  this  model 
by  creating  a  hypothetical  100-g  worm  that  has  5  ml  of 
hemolymph  and  15  g  of  trophosome  (Childress  el  ai, 
1984).  At  200  juM  external  2H2S,  one  would  expect  5.9 
mM  2H2S,  5  mM  O2,  and  2  mM  SCO2  in  the  hemo- 
lymph. At  an  uptake  rate  of  5  ^mole  2H2S  g~'rr',  the 
O2  uptake  rate  would  be  8  j/mole  g~  'h~ '  and  the  net  2CO2 
uptake  would  be  2  j/mole  g~'  h~'.  If  one  assumes  that  the 
hemolymph  makes  one  circuit  per  minute,  one  can  cal- 
culate that  27%  of  the  2H2S,  53%  of  the  O2,  and  33%  of 
the  2CO2  must  be  exchanged  on  each  circuit.  These 
numbers  are  not  unreasonable,  while  at  the  same  time 
the  similarity  of  the  percentages  provides  some  confidence 
that  the  values  used  for  the  hemolymph  concentrations 
are  approximately  correct. 

Acknowledgments 

This  work  was  supported  by  NSF  grants  OCE-8609202 
and  OCE-90 12076  to  J.J.C.  and  OCE-8610514  to  J.J.C. 
and  C.R.F.  and  funding  from  IFREMER  to  A.-M.  Alayse. 
Some  travel  support  was  provided  by  NATO  grant 
D.880423  to  H.  Felbeck.  We  would  like  to  thank  R.  Van 
Buskirk,  V.  Vanderveer,  and  D.  Gage  for  technical  assis- 
tance during  the  cruises.  Thanks  are  also  due  to  the  cap- 
tains and  crews  of  the  R}'  Melville,  R  V  Thomas  Thomp- 
son, and  N/O  Nadir,  as  well  as  the  sub  crews  and  pilots 


152 


J.  J.  CH1LDRESS  ET  AL. 


of  the  submersibles  Alvin  and  Nautile,  without  whom  this 
work  would  not  have  been  possible.  This  manuscript  has 
benefited  from  discussions  with  and  comments  by  A.  An- 
derson, H.  Felbeck,  R.  Trench,  and  J.  Feigenbaum. 

Literature  Cited 

Anderson,  A.  E.,  J.  J.  Childress,  and  J.  Favuzzi.  1987.  Net  uptake  of 
CO2  driven  by  sulfide  and  thiosulfate  oxidation  in  the  bacterial  sym- 
biont-containing  clam  Solcmya  reidi.  J.  Exp.  Biol.  133:  1-31. 

Arp,  A.  J.,  and  J.  J.  Childress.  1981.  Blood  function  in  the  hydro- 
thermal  vent  vestimentiferan  tube  worm.  Science  213:  342-344. 

Arp,  A.  J.,  and  J.  J.  Childress.  1983.  Sulfide  binding  by  the  blood  of 
the  hydrothermal  vent  tube  worm  Ri/iia  pachvplila  Science  219: 
295-297. 

Arp,  A.  J.,  J.  J.  Childress,  and  C.  R.  Fisher  Jr.  1985.  Blood  gas  transport 
in  Riftia  pachyptila.  Bull.  Biol.  Sue.  Hash.  6:  289-300. 

Arp,  A.  J.,  J.  J.  Childress,  and  R.  D.  Vetter.  1987.  The  sulfide  binding 
protein  in  the  blood  of  Ri/iia  pachvplila  is  the  extracellular  hemo- 
globin. J.  Exp.  Biol.  128:  139-158. 

Belkin,  S.,  D.  C.  Nelson,  and  H.  \V  .  Jannasch.  1986.  Symbiotic  assim- 
ilation of  CO2  in  two  hydrothermal  vent  animals,  the  mussel  Bathy- 
modiolus  thermophilus  and  the  tube  worm  Riftia  pachvptila.  Biol. 
Bull  170:  110-121. 

Ca»anaugh,  C.  M.  1985.  Symbiosis  of  chemoautotrophic  bacteria  and 
marine  invertebrates  from  hydrothermal  vents  and  reducing  sedi- 
ments. Bull.  Biol.  Sot:  Wash.  6:  373-388. 

Cavanaugh,  C.  M.,  S.  L.  Gardiner,  M.  L.  Jones,  H.  \V.  Jannasch,  and 
J.  B.  Waterbury.  1981 .  Prokaryotic  cells  in  the  hydrothermal  vent 
tube  worm  Riftia  pachvptila:  possible  chemoautotrophic  symbionts. 
Science  213:  340-342. 

Childress,  J.  J.  1988.  Biology  and  chemistry  of  a  deep-sea  hydrothermal 
vent  on  the  Galapagos  Rift;  the  Rose  Garden  in  1985,  an  introduction. 
Deep-Sea  Res  35:  1677-1680. 

Childress,  J.  J.,  A.  J.  Arp,  and  C.  R.  Fisher  Jr.  1984.  Metabolic  and 
blood  characteristics  of  the  hydrothermal  vent  tube  worm  Rijiia  pa- 
chvptila. Mar.  Biol.  83:  109-124. 

Childress,  J.  J.,  C.  R.  Fisher,  J.  A.  Favuzzi,  and  N.  K.  Sanders. 
1991 .  Factors  affecting  the  uptake  of  sulfide  and  carbon  dioxide  by 
the  hydrothermal  vent  clam,  Calyptitgena  magnijica.  Phvsiol.  Zool. 
In  press. 

Degens,  E.  T.,  R.  R.  L.  Guillard,  W.  M.  Sackett,  and  A.  Hellebust. 
1968.  Metabolic  fractionation  of  carbon  isotopes  in  marine  plank- 
ton— I.  Temperature  and  respiration  experiments.  Deep-Sea  Res 
15:  1-9. 

Fahey,  R.  C,  R.  D.  Dorian,  G.  L.  Newton,  and  J.  Utley. 
1983.  Determination  of  intracellular  thiol  levels  using  monobro- 
mobimane  fluorescent  labeling:  applications  involving  radioprotective 
drugs.  Pp.  103-120  in  Radioproleclors  and  Carcinogens.  O.  F.  Ny- 
gaard,  and  M.  G.  Simie,  ed.  Academic  Press,  New  York. 

Felbeck,  H.  1981.  Chemoautotrophic  potential  of  the  hydrothermal 
vent  tube  worm,  Riftia  pachyptila  Jones  (Vestimentifera).  Science 
213:  336-338. 

Felbeck,  H.  1985.  CO2  fixation  in  the  hydrothermal  vent  tube  worm 
Riftia  pachyptila  (Jones).  Physiol.  Zool.  53:  272-281. 

Felbeck,  H.,  and  J.  J.  Childress.  1988.  Rijiia  pachyptila:  a  highly  in- 
tegrated symbiosis.  Oceanol.  Ada  Special  I'ol.  8:  131-138. 

Felbeck,  H.,  G.  N.  Somero,  and  J.  J.  Childress.  1981.  Calvin-Benson 
cycle  sulphide  oxidation  enzymes  in  animals  from  sulphide  rich  hab- 
itats. Nature  293:  291-293. 

Fisher,  C.  R.,  Jr.,  and  J.  J.  Childress.  1984.  Substrate  oxidation  by 
trophosome  tissue  from  Riftia  pachyptila  Jones  (Phylum  Pogono- 
phora).  Mar.  Biol.  Lett  5:  171-184. 


Fisher,  C.  R.,  and  J.  J.  Childress.  1986.  Translocation  of  fixed  carbon 
from  symbiotic  bacteria  to  host  tissues  in  the  gutless  bivalve,  Solemva 
reidi.  Mar.  Biol  93:  59-68. 

Fisher,  C.  R.,  J.  J.  Childress,  and  N.  K.  Sanders.  I988a.  The  role  of 
vestimentiferan  hemoglobin  in  providing  an  environment  suitable 
for  chemoautotrophic  sulfide-oxidizing  endosymbionts.  Symbiosis 
5:  229-246. 

Fisher,  C.  R.,  J.  J.  Childress,  A.  J.  Arp.  J.  M.  Brooks,  D.  Distel,  J.  A. 
Favuzzi,  S.  A.  Macko,  A.  Newton,  M.  A.  Powell,  G.  N.  Somero,  and 
T.  Soto.  I988b.  Physiology,  morphology,  and  composition  of  Ri/iia 
pachyptila  at  Rose  Garden  in  1985.  Deep-Sea  Res  35:  1745-1758. 
Fisher,  C.  R.,  J.  J.  Childress,  A.  J.  Arp,  J.  M.  Brooks,  D.  Distel,  J.  A. 
Favuzzi,  H.  Felbeck,  R.  Messier,  K.  S.  Johnson,  M.  C.  Kennicutt  II, 
S.  A.  Macko,  A.  Newton,  M.  A.  Powell,  G.  N.  Somero,  and  T.  Soto. 
1988c.  Microhabitat  variation  in  the  hydrothermal  vent  mussel 
Bathymodiolus  thermophilus,  at  Rose  Garden  vent  on  the  Galapagos 
rift.  Deep-Sea  Res.  35:  1769-1792. 

Fisher,  C.  R.,  J.  J.  Childress,  and  J.  M.  Brooks.  1988d.     Are  hydro- 
thermal-vent  vestimentifera  carbon-limited?  Am.  Zool.  28:  128A. 
Fisher,  C.  R.,  J.  J.  Childress,  and  E.  Minnich.  1989.     Autotrophic  carbon 
fixation  by  the  chemoautotrophic  symbionts  of  Riftia  pachvptila. 
Biol.  Bull.  177:  372-385. 

Fisher,  C.  R.,  M.  C.  Kennicutt  II,  and  J.  M.  Brooks.  1990.  Stable 
carbon  isotopic  evidence  for  carbon  limitation  in  hydrothermal  vent 
vestimentiferans.  Science  247:  1094-1096. 

Fustec,  A.,  D.  Desbruyeres,  and  L.  Laubier.  1988.     Biomass  estimation 
of  animal  communities  associated  with  deep-sea  hydrothermal  vents 
near  13°N/EPR.  Oceanol.  Ada  Spec.  I'ol.  8:  15-22. 
Groenendaal,  M.  1981.     The  adaptation  ofArenico/a  marina  to  sulphide 

solutions.  Neth.  J.  Sea  Res.  15:  65-77. 

Heisler.  N.  1984.  Acid-base  regulation  in  fishes.  Pp.  315-392  in  Fish 
Physiology.  W.  S.  Hoar,  and  D.  J.  Randall,  ed.  Academic  Press,  Or- 
lando. 

Hessler,  R.  R.,  W.  M.  Smithey,  M.  A.  Boudrias,  C.  H.  Keller,  R.  A. 

Lutz,  and  J.  J.  Childress.  1988.     Temporal  change  in  megafauna  at 

the  Rose  Garden  hydrothermal  vent.  Deep-Sea  Res.  35:  1681-1710. 

Jaques,  A.  G.  1936.     The  kinetics  of  penetration:  XII.  Hydrogen  sulfide 

J.  Gen.  Physiol.  19:397-418. 

Johnson,  K.  S.,  J.  J.  Childress,  and  C.  L.  Beehler.  1988a.  Short  term 
temperature  variability  in  the  Rose  Garden  hydrothermal  vent  field. 
Deep-Sea  Res  35:  171 1-1722. 

Johnson,  K.  S.,  C.  M.  Sakamoto-Arnold,  J.  J.  Childress,  and  C.  L.  Beeh- 
ler. 1988b.  The  biogeochemistry  of  hydrothermal  vent  communities 
in  the  Galapagos  Rift.  Eos  69:  1271. 

Jones,  M.  L.  1981.     Ri/iia  pachyptila,  new  genus,  new  species,  the  ves- 
timentiferan tubeworm  from  the  Galapagos  Rift  geothermal  vents. 
Pmc.  Biol.  Sot:  Hash.  93:  1295-1313. 
Jones,  M.  L.  1988.     The  Vestimentifera.  their  biology,  systematic  and 

evolutionary  patterns.  Oceanol  Ada  Spec   Vol.  8:  69-82. 
Jones,  M.  L.,  and  S.  L.  Gardiner.  1988.     Evidence  for  a  transient  diges- 
tive tract  in  Vestimentifera.  Prot:  Biol.  Sot:  Wash.  101:  423-433. 
Kelly,  D.  P.  1982.     Biochemistry  of  the  chemolithotrophic  oxidation 
of  inorganic  sulphur.  Phil.  Trans.  ROY.  Sot:  Loud.  Ser.  B  298:  499- 
528. 

Mason,  J.,  D.  P.  Kelly,  and  A.  P.  Wood.  1987.  Chemolithotrophic  and 
autotrophic  growth  of  Thermothrix  thiopara  and  some  thiobacilli  on 
thiosulphate  and  polythionates,  and  a  reassessment  of  the  growth 
yields  of  T  thiopara  in  chemostat  culture.  /  (Jen.  Microbiol.  133: 
1249-1256. 
Millero,  F.  J.  1986.  The  thermodynamics  and  kinetics  of  the  hydrogen 

sulfide  system  in  natural  waters.  Mar.  Chem.  18:  121-147. 
Millero,  F.  J.,  T.  Plese,  and  M.  Fernandez.  1988.     The  dissociation  of 
hydrogen  sulfide  in  seawater.  Limnol.  Oceanogr  33:  269-274. 


AUTOTROPH1C  FUNCTION  IN  Rlb'TIA 


153 


Nelson,  D.  C,  B.  B.  Jorgensen,  and  N.  P.  Rcvsbach.  1986.  Growth 
pattern  and  yield  of  a  chemoautotrophic  Beggiatoa  sp.  in  oxygen- 
sulfide  microgradients.  Appl  Environ  Murohiol  52:  225-233. 

Newton,  G.  L.,  R.  Dorian,  and  R.  C.  Fahey.  1981.  Analysis  of  biological 
thiols  by  derivatization  with  monobromobimane  and  separation  by 
reverse-phase  high  performance  liquid  chromatography.  Amil 
Biochem.  114:  383-387. 

O'Brien,  J..  and  R.  D.  Vetter.  1990.  Production  of  thiosulfate  during 
sulphide  oxidation  by  mitochondria  of  the  symbiom-containing  bi- 
valve Sulemya  reidi.  J  Exp.  Biol.  149:  133-148. 

Powell,  M.  A.,  and  G.  N.  Somero.  1983.  Blood  components  prevent 
sulfide  poisoning  of  respiration  of  the  hydrothermal  vent  tubeworm 
Riftiii  paehyptila.  Science  219:  297-299. 

Powell,  M.  A.,  and  G.  N.  Somero.  1985.  Sulfide  oxidation  occurs  in 
the  animal  tissue  of  the  gutless  clam.  Solemya  reidi.  Binl.  Bull  169: 
164-181. 

Powell,  M.  A.,  and  G.  N.  Somero.  1986.  Adaptations  to  sulfide  by 
hydrothermal  vent  animals:  sites  and  mechanisms  of  detoxification 
and  metabolism.  Biol.  Bull  171:  274-290. 

Quetin,  I..  B.,  and  J.  J.  Childress.  1980.  Observations  on  the  swimming 
activity  of  two  bathypelagic  mysid  species  maintained  at  high  hy- 
drostatic pressures.  Deep-Sea  Res  27  A:  383-391. 

Rau,  G.  H.  1981.  Hydrothermal  vent  clam  and  tubeworm  I3C/I2C. 
Further  evidence  of  nonphotosynthetic  food  sources.  Science  213: 
338_340. 

Rau,  G.  H.,  and  J.  I.  Hedges.  1979.  Carbon-13  depletion  in  a  hydro- 
thermal  vent  mussel:  suggestion  of  a  chemosynthetic  food  source. 
Science  203:  648-649. 

Richard,  J.  J.,  R.  D.  Vick,  and  G.  A.  Junk.  1977.  Determination  of 
elemental  sulfur  by  gas  chromatography.  Env  Sci.  and  Techno!.  11: 
1084-1086. 

Roux,  M.,  M.  Rio,  E.  Schein,  R.  Lutz,  L.  Fritz,  and  L.  Ragone. 
1989.  Mesures  in  situ  de  la  croissance  des  bivalves  et  des  vesti- 
mentiferes  et  de  la  corrosion  des  coquilles  au  site  hydrothermal  de 
13°N  (dorsale  du  Pacinque  oriental).  C  R  Acad.  Sci.  Paris.  Ser  III 
308:  121-127. 


Skirrow,  G.  1975.  The  dissolved  gases— carbon  dioxide.  P.  1  in  Chem- 
ical Oceanography.  J.  P.  Riley.  and  G.  Skirrow,  ed.  Academic  Press, 
New  York. 

Somero,  G.  N.,  J.  J.  Childress,  and  A.  E.  Anderson.  1989.  Transport, 
metabolism  and  detoxification  of  hydrogen  sulfide  in  animals  from 
sulfide  nch  marine  environments.  Crit.  Rev  Aqual.  Sci.  1:  591-614. 

Southward,  E.  C.  1987.  Contribution  of  symbiotic  chemoautotrophs 
to  the  nutrition  of  benthic  invertebrates.  Pp.  83-1 18  in  Microbes  in 
the  Sea.  M  A.  Sleigh,  ed.  Horwood  Ltd.,  Chichester. 

Southward,  E.  C.  1988.  Development  of  the  gut  and  segmentation  of 
newly  settled  stages  of  Ridgeiu  (Vestimentifera):  implications  for  re- 
lationship between  Vestimentifera  and  Pogonophora.  /.  Mar.  Biol. 
As.wc.  U.  A  68:  465-487. 

Terwilliger,  R.  C.,  and  N.  B.  Terwilliger.  1985.  Respiratory  proteins 
of  hydrothermal  vent  animals.  Bull  Biol.  Soc.  H'axh.  6:  273-288. 

Terwilliger,  R.  C.,  N.  B.  Terwilliger,  and  E.  Schabtach.  1980.  The 
structure  of  hemoglobin  from  an  unusual  deep-sea  worm  (Vesti- 
mentifera). Camp.  Biochcm.  Physiol.  65B:  531-535. 

Toulmond,  A.  1977.  Temperature-induced  variations  of  blood  acid- 
base  status  in  the  lugworm.  Arenicola  marina  (L.):  I.  In  vitro  study. 
Rcspir.  P/n-siol  31:  139-149. 

Vetter,  R.  D.  1985.  Elemental  sulfur  in  the  gills  of  three  species  of 
clams  containing  chemoautrophic  symbiotic  bacteria:  a  possible  in- 
organic energy  storage  compound.  Mar  Biol  88:  33-42. 

Vetter,  R.  D.,  P.  A.  Matrai,  B.  Javor,  and  J.  O'Brien.  1989.  Reduced 
sulfur  compounds  in  the  marine  environment:  analysis  by  HPLC. 
Pp.  244-26 1  in  Biogenic  Siillin  in  the  Environment.  E.  S.  Saltzman. 
and  W.  J.  Cooper,  ed.  American  Chemical  Society,  Washington. 

Vetter,  R.  D.,  M.  E.  Wells,  A.  L.  Kurtsman,  and  G.  N.  Somero. 
1987.  Sulfide  detoxification  by  the  hydrothermal  vent  crab  Bytho- 
graea  ihermvdron  and  other  decapod  crustaceans.  Physiol.  Zool.  60: 
121-137. 

VVillason,  S.  VV.,  and  K.  S.  Johnson.  1986.  A  rapid,  highly  sensitive 
technique  for  the  determination  of  ammonia  in  seawater.  Afar.  Biol 
91:  285-290. 

\\  ilmot,  D.  B.  J.,  and  R.  D.  Vetter.  1990.  The  bacterial  symbiont  from 
the  hydrothermal  vent  tubeworm  Riliia  paehyptila  is  a  sulfide  spe- 
cialist. Alar.  Biol.  106:  273-283. 


Reference:  Bin!  Bull  180:  154-166.  (February.  ll»91) 


Distribution  and  Characterization  of  Ion  Transporting 

and  Respiratory  Filaments  in  the  Gills 

of  Procambarus  clarkii 


JOHN  S.  DICKSON,  RICHARD  M.  DILLAMAN, 
ROBERT  D.  ROER,  AND  DAVID  B.  ROYE 

Center  for  Marine  Science  Research,  University  of  North  Carolina  at  Wilmington, 
7205  n'rightsvi/le  Ave.,  Wilmington,  North  Carolina  28403 


Abstract.  Individual  gill  filaments  of  the  freshwater 
crayfish  Procambarus  clarkii  were  determined  to  be  either 
predominantly  respiratory  or  transporting.  Silver  staining 
revealed  that  the  filaments  within  the  central  bed  of  the 
gills  formed  silver  deposits  whereas  filaments  at  the  mar- 
gins and  the  entire  sixth  pleurobranch  formed  no  deposits. 
Designation  of  the  silver  staining  gills  as  predominantly 
transporting  and  unstained  filaments  as  predominantly 
respiratory  was  substantiated  by  ultrastructural  analyses 
and  measurements  of  ATPase  and  transepithelial  poten- 
tials. Presumptive  transporting  filaments  had  an  epithe- 
lium subjacent  to  the  cuticle  that  was  relatively  thick  and 
dominated  by  abundant  mitochondria.  Lacunae  were  de- 
lineated by  pillar  structures  and  served  as  collateral  path- 
ways for  the  movement  of  blood  from  the  afferent  to  ef- 
ferent blood  channels,  which  were  separated  by  a  thin 
septum.  Presumptive  respiratory  filaments  had  an  ex- 
tremely thin  epithelium  with  few  organelles,  but  a  rela- 
tively thick  septum.  Present  in  both  types  of  filaments 
were  nerves  and  podocytes.  The  values  for  Na,  K-ATPase 
were  significantly  higher  in  the  transporting  filaments  than 
in  those  designated  as  respiratory.  The  measurement  of 
transepithelial  potentials  showed  both  filaments  to  be  cat- 
ion selective  with  the  respiratory  filaments  slightly  more 
positive  and  the  transporting  filaments  slightly  more  neg- 
ative than  the  diffusion  potential  for  Na. 

Introduction 

Gills  are  a  site  for  exchange  between  the  blood  of  the 
organism  and  the  external  medium  and,  as  such,  are  in- 
Received  28  June  1990;  accepted  9  November  1990. 


volved  in  the  separate  but  interrelated  functions  of  gas 
exchange,  acid/base  balance,  and  ion  regulation.  Those 
substances  exchanged  may  include  oxygen,  electrolytes, 
carbon  dioxide,  and  ammonia.  To  produce  a  large  surface 
area  for  such  exchange,  gills  are  often  branched  along  one 
or  more  axes.  In  the  crabs  (Crustacea,  Decapoda)  lamellar 
gills  are  present  (Copeland,  1963,  1968;  Copeland  and 
Fitzjarrell.  1968;  Taylor  and  Greenaway,  1979;  Finol  and 
Croghan,  1983)  and  consist  of  a  parallel  afferent  and  ef- 
ferent blood  vessel  between  which  are  broad  flattened 
sheets.  In  branchipod  crustaceans,  such  Artemia  salina 
and  Daphnia  nuigna  (Copeland,  1967  and  Kikuchi,  1983), 
amphipods  and  isopods  (Milne  and  Ellis,  1973;  Bubel  and 
Jones,  1974)  gills  are  flat,  oval,  sac-like  extensions  of  tho- 
racic appendages  or  modified  pleopods.  In  crayfish  and 
shrimp  the  gills  are  filamentous  (Morse  el  a/..  1970;  Bie- 
lawski,  1971;  Fisher,  1972;  Burggren  el  ai.  1974;  Foster 
and  Howse,  1 978).  In  crayfish  the  gills  are  trichobranchiate 
and  have  a  central  stalk  from  which  hundreds  of  tiny 
finger-like  filaments  arise.  The  podobranchia  (outermost 
gills)  are  attached  to  the  coxae  of  the  appendages.  A  mem- 
branous lamina,  devoid  of  filaments,  extends  from  the 
inner  side  of  the  podobranch  and  lies  against  the  thoracic 
body  wall;  its  distal  tip  is  flattened  into  a  plate  bearing  a 
few  filaments.  The  arthrobranchia,  which  are  attached  to 
the  articular  membrane  between  the  body  wall  and  the 
appendages,  and  the  pleurobranchia,  which  are  attached 
to  the  pleural  wall  of  each  somite,  lie  beneath  the  podob- 
ranchia and  have  no  distal  plate  structure  (Huxley,  1986; 
Burggren  et  ai,  1974). 

Despite  variations  in  gill  anatomy,  the  ultrastructure 
of  the  gill  epithelium  in  those  species  that  are  capable  of 
osmoregulation  is  similar,  and  can  be  placed  into  one  of 


154 


GILL  FILAMENTS  IN  P    Cl.ARkll 


155 


two  categories.  Both  categories  consist  of  a  single  layered 
epithelium  that  separates  blood  spaces  from  the  uncal- 
cified  cuticle.  One  type  of  epithelium  is  relatively  thick 
and  has  the  characteristics  of  an  ion  transporting  epithe- 
lium (seeCioffi.  1984,  for  a  review  of  arthropod  ion  trans- 
porting epithelia).  Briefly,  it  consists  of  cells  with  highly 
folded  apical  and  basal  processes,  a  prominent  basal  lam- 
ina and  cytoplasm  containing  abundant  mitochondria  as 
well  as  golgi  apparati,  smooth  and  rough  endoplasmic 
reticulum  and  glycogen.  The  second  type  of  epithelium 
consists  of  very  thin  squamous  cells  subjacent  to  the  cu- 
ticle. The  cells  are  only  one  tenth  the  thickness  of  the 
previously  described  type,  and  the  cytoplasm  contains  very 
few  organelles  (Copeland  and  Fitzjarrell,  1968).  This  ul- 
trastructure  would  appear  to  favor  diffusion  of  gases  be- 
tween the  external  and  internal  media  and  consequently 
this  epithelium  has  been  categorized  as  being  respiratory 
in  function. 

Studies  on  a  variety  of  species  of  decapod  crustaceans 
have  demonstrated  the  existence  of  a  partitioning  of  ion 
regulatory  and  respiratory  functions  among  different  gills 
or  within  different  regions  of  gills.  Both  morphological 
and  biochemical  (Na,  K-ATPase  distribution)  studies  have 
provided  evidence  that  the  anterior  gills  are  dedicated  to 
gas  exchange  while  the  posterior  gills  are  involved  in  ion 
regulation  in  the  euryhaline  brachyurans  Eriocheir  xinen- 
sis  (Pequeux  and  Gilles,  1978,  1981),  Uca  pugnax  (Hol- 
liday,  1985),  Uca  /n/miv  (Wanson  etal..  \984),Callinectes 
sapidus  (Copeland  and  Fitzjarrell,  1968;  Neufeld  et  al., 
1980;  Towle,  1984),  and  Carcinus  maenas  (Mantel  and 
Landesman,  1977).  Although  we  and  other  investigators 
realize  that  gill  tissues  possessing  transport  epithelia  are 
also  engaged  in  gas  exchange,  and  that  ions  move  across 
respiratory  surfaces,  we  still  use.  for  simplicity,  the  pre- 
dominant function  to  designate  that  of  the  entire  filament. 
Henceforth,  we  will  refer  to  those  gill  filaments  that  possess 
a  typical  transporting  epithelium  as  "transport"  types  and 
those  gill  filaments  with  a  squamous  epithelium  as  "re- 
spiratory" types. 

Crayfish,  like  the  euryhaline  brachyurans  in  dilute  me- 
dia, exist  in  a  hypoosmotic  medium,  and  must  maintain 
their  ion  balance  by  actively  transporting  electrolytes 
across  the  gills  (Maluf,  1940;  Bryan,  1959;  Shaw,  1960a. 
b;  Ehrenfeld,  1974).  Unlike  the  euryhaline  brachyurans 
studied,  however,  most  crayfish  are  stenohaline  freshwater 
crustaceans  and  possess  filamentous,  rather  than  lamellate, 
gills.  The  partitioning  of  respiratory  and  ion  transporting 
function  in  the  gills  of  crayfish  is  still  in  question.  While 
Wheatly  and  Henry  (1987)  have  used  biochemical  anal- 
yses of  pooled  gill  sets  to  show  that  the  distribution  of  Na, 
K-ATPase  was  similar  among  gills  of  Pacifastacus  len- 
iusculus,  Morse  et  al.  (1970)  used  silver  staining  and  re- 
ported a  partitioning  of  respiratory  and  transport  functions 
among  the  gill  filaments  of  the  same  species.  Dunel-Erb 


el  al.  ( 1982).  on  the  other  hand,  observed  only  respiratory 
epithelial  tissues  in  the  filaments  and  ion  transporting  ep- 
ithelial tissues  in  the  podobranch  lamina  of  Astacus  pal- 
lipes  and  therefore  assigned  the  appropriate  function  to 
the  two  structures.  The  only  ultrastructural  observations 
of  filaments  have  been  of  transporting  epithelia  (Morse 
et  al..  1970;  Burggren  et  al..  1974;  Bielawski,  1971;  Fisher, 
1972). 

This  investigation  was  therefore  directed  at  determin- 
ing— within  a  single  filament,  within  a  single  gill,  and 
among  gills — the  distribution  and  frequency  of  the  two 
types  of  epithelia  of  the  freshwater  crayfish  Procambarus 
chirkii.  The  methods  employed  in  this  study  were  silver 
staining  (Maluf,  1940),  an  indicator  of  ion  transporting 
tissue;  transmission  electron  microscopy  (TEM),  which 
reveals  the  striking  difference  between  the  morphology  of 
the  two  types  of  epithelia;  analysis  for  Na,  K-ATPase,  an 
effector  of  ion  movement;  and  measurement  of  transep- 
ithelial  potentials  in  individual  filaments,  which  reflects 
possible  differences  in  transport  or  permeability  of  the 
combined  epithelial  and  cuticular  layers. 

Materials  and  Methods 

The  crayfish  (Procambarus  clarkii),  obtained  from 
Carolina  Biological  Supply,  were  kept  in  aquaria  of  arti- 
ficial pond  water  (Roer  and  Shelton,  1982),  maintained 
between  20°C  and  25°C,  and  fed  2-3  times  a  week.  Only 
active  crayfish  in  the  intermolt  stage,  as  determined  by 
the  criteria  of  Drach  and  Tchernigovtzeff  ( 1967)  and  Ste- 
venson ( 1972),  were  used  in  this  study. 

Silver  staining 

Crayfish  subjected  to  silver  staining  were  first  rinsed  in 
deionized  water  and  placed  in  a  0.05%  AgNO3  solution 
at  25°C  for  30  min.  The  volume  of  the  solution  was  suf- 
ficient to  completely  submerge  the  crayfish.  Crayfish  were 
then  rinsed  with  deionized  water.  The  branchiostegites 
were  removed  to  expose  the  gills,  and  the  crayfish  were 
placed  in  Kodak  Microdol-X  developer  (diluted  3:1  to 
lower  the  osmolarity  to  630  mOsm)  for  one  hour  with 
frequent  agitation.  After  the  gills  were  thoroughly  washed 
and  placed  in  distilled  water,  one  or  two  drops  of  NH4S 
were  added  to  intensify  the  stain. 

Preparation  oj  gill  filament  tissues 
for  electron  microscopy 

Silver  treated  filaments  were  fixed  in  3%  glutaraldehyde 
in  0. 1  M  cacodylate  buffer  (pH  7.2)  containing  5%  sucrose 
(osmolarity  =  640  mOsm)  at  25 °C  for  at  least  1  h.  To 
facilitate  penetration  of  the  various  solutions,  the  tips  were 
cut  off"  the  filaments  and  the  tissue  was  gently  agitated  at 
each  step  of  the  preparation.  The  filaments  were  then 


156 


J.  S.  DICKSON  ET  AL 


rinsed  in  buffer  and  postfixed  with  2%  osmium  tetroxide 
in  0.1  M  cacodylate  buffer  (pH  7.2)  plus  5%  sucrose  at 
25°C  for  2  h.  After  another  rinse  with  buffer,  tissues  were 
dehydrated  with  a  graded  series  of  ethanol  and  propylene 
oxide,  and  embedded  in  Spurr  low  viscosity  embedding 
medium  (Spurr,  1969). 

Filaments  not  silver-treated  were  fixed  in  2%  glutaral- 
dehyde  in  0.15  M  cacodylate  buffer,  pH  7.4,  with  5  m.M 
CaCl2  for  1  h,  rinsed  in  buffer,  and  post-fixed  in  fresh 
0.5%  OsO4  with  0.8%  K4Fe(CN)6  in  0.1  M  cacodylate 
buffer,  pH  7.4,  with  5  mA/CaC!2  at  25°C  for  1  h.  After 
rinsing  again  in  cacodylate  buffer,  the  tissues  were  placed 
in  0. 1 5%  tannic  acid  in  0. 1 5  M  cacodylate  buffer,  pH  7.4, 
with  5  mA/  CaCl2  for  5  min.  Following  a  short  rinse  in 
buffer  and  distilled  water,  the  tissues  were  stained  en  bloc 
with  2%  aqueous  uranyl  acetate  for  2  h.  After  a  distilled 
water  rinse,  tissues  were  dehydrated  through  a  graded  se- 
ries of  acetone  and  embedded  in  Spurr. 

Cross-sections  were  cut  in  the  basal,  medial,  and  distal 
regions  of  the  filaments.  Sections  were  post-stained  with 
4%  uranyl  acetate  in  50%  ethanol  and  Reynolds  lead  ci- 
trate (Reynolds,  1963),  and  were  viewed  with  a  Zeiss  EM 
9S  transmission  electron  microscope  operated  at  60  kV. 

Na,  K-A  TPase  assay  of  filaments 

Na,  K-ATPase  assays  were  done  according  to  the 
method  of  Horiuchi  (1977).  After  washing  excised  gills  in 
0.25  M  sucrose  buffered  to  pH  7.5  with  100  mAl  Tris- 
HC1,  predicted  transporting  or  respiratory  filaments  were 
removed  from  the  gills,  pooled  by  type,  and  homogenized 
at  4°C  in  2  volumes  of  the  buffered  sucrose  solution  during 
35  passes  in  a  glass  tissue  homogenizer.  The  homogenate 
was  centrifuged  at  3000  X  g  for  15  min  and  the  super- 
natant was  assayed  for  activity. 

The  reaction  mixture  consisted  of  60  mA/  NaCl.  20 
mA/  KC1,  2  mA/  MgCl2  and  30  mA/  Tris-HCl,  pH  7.5, 
in  0.8  ml.  This  reaction  mixture  was  preincubated  at  32°C 
before  0. 1  ml  of  the  enzyme  solution  and  0. 1  ml  of  the 
3.0  mA/  Tris-ATP  were  added.  The  enzymatic  reaction 
was  allowed  to  proceed  for  45  min,  being  terminated  by 
the  addition  of  cold  3%  trichloroacetic  acid.  The  amount 
of  inorganic  phosphate,  hydrolyzed  from  ATP,  present  in 
the  supernatant  was  measured  according  to  Wheeler 
( 1975).  Mg-ATPase  activity  was  determined  as  above  with 
the  substitution  of  10  mA/  ouabain  for  KC1  in  the  reaction 
mixture  to  inhibit  Na,  K-ATPase.  Na,  K-ATPase  activity 
was  calculated  as  the  difference  between  the  total  ATPase 
and  Mg-ATPase  values.  The  amount  of  protein  was  de- 
termined according  to  Peterson's  (1977)  modified  Lowry 
protein  assay  using  a  Bausch  and  Lomb  Spectronic  710 
spectrophotometer  and  bovine  serum  albumin  as  a  stan- 
dard. The  specific  activities  were  recorded  as  micromoles 
of  inorganic  phosphate  per  milligram  of  protein  per  hour. 


Transepithetial  potential  measurements 

Crayfish  were  submerged  in  'Xi  strength  Van  Harreveld's 
Ringer  solution  (final  concentrations:  NaCl  =  51.3  mA/; 
KC1  =  1.3  mA/;  CaCl:  =  3.4  mA/;  MgCl2  =  0.3  mA/)  in 
a  finger  bowl,  and  restrained  by  rubber  bands  attached  to 
a  notched  plexiglass  plate.  Diluted  Ringer's  solution  was 
used  to  provide  sufficient  electrolytes  for  electrode  con- 
duction. Potentials  measured  with  pond  water  as  the  ex- 
ternal medium  were  unreliable.  The  branchial  region  of 
the  carapace  was  cut  away  from  one  side  of  the  animal 
to  expose  the  gills.  In  this  position,  individual  gill  filaments 
could  easily  be  impaled  by  glass  microelectrodes  using  a 
micromanipulator. 

Microelectrodes  were  drawn  from  glass  capillary  tubing 
and  filled  with  3  A/  KC1.  A  Ag-AgCl  reference  electrode 
was  immersed  in  the  bathing  medium.  Both  electrodes 
were  connected  by  shielded  cables  to  a  WPI  model  701 
microprobe  system  and  a  Tectronix  5111  storage  oscil- 
loscope. 

Only  filaments  of  the  podobranchs  were  impaled  and 
the  assignment  of  putative  transport  and  respiratory  fil- 
aments was  based  upon  the  results  of  the  silver  staining 
experiments.  Transepithelial  potentials  (TEP's)  and  the 
position  of  the  electrode  tip  within  the  filament  were  re- 
corded each  time  a  gill  was  impaled.  Sodium  concentra- 
tions of  the  medium  and  crayfish  hemolymph  were  mea- 
sured by  flame  photometry  (Turner  model  510). 


Results 


Silver  staining 


In  six  crayfish  subjected  to  silver  staining  all  showed 
some  staining  in  all  gills  except  pleurobranch  6.  Staining 
occurred  only  in  the  filaments.  That  is,  no  staining  was 
observed  in  the  central  gill  stalk,  in  the  lamina,  in  the 
distal  plate  of  the  podobranchia,  nor  at  the  base  of  the 
gills.  Figure  1  is  representative  of  the  filament  staining 
patterns.  The  podobranchs  had  the  largest  proportion  of 
stained  filaments,  whereas  the  pleurobranchs  had  the  least 
for  each  set  of  gills.  All  gills  showed  the  same  basic  pattern, 
a  population  of  stained  filaments  in  the  central  area  of 
the  filament  bed  surrounded  by  lateral  rows  of  unstained 
filaments,  with  the  exception  of  the  pleurobranch  of  the 
sixth  gill  set,  which  showed  no  silver  staining.  Silver 
stained  filaments  had  precipitate  distributed  evenly  over 
the  length  of  the  filament. 

Transmission  electron  microscopy  of  silver  stained  fil- 
aments showed  that  most  of  the  precipitate  was  within 
the  gill  cuticle,  although  lesser  amounts  were  observed 
outside  or  beneath  the  cuticle  (Fig.  2).  Precipitate  within 
the  cuticle  was  localized  almost  exclusively  in  a  layer  cor- 
responding to  the  exocuticle,  rarely  being  found  in  the 
epicuticle  or  endocuticle.  While  the  treatment  employed 


GILL  FILAMENTS  IN  P.  CLIRKII 


157 


Podob ranch 


Arthrobranch 


Pleurobranch 


Gill  Set 


Gill  Set  #2 


GUI  Set  #3 


Gill  Set 


Gill  Set 


Gill  Set  #6 


Figure  I.     Light  micrographs  of  six  sets  of  gills  from  the  right  side  of  a  crayfish  after  silver  staining.  Each 
gill  is  oriented  with  the  base  to  the  right.  Dark  staining  indicates  silver  deposition. 


in  the  silver  staining  disrupted  the  soft  tissues,  it  was  pos- 
sible to  note  that  the  epithelium  underlying  the  cuticle 
was  relatively  thick  and  had  numerous  basal  and  apical 
infoldings.  Filaments  containing  no  precipitate  after  silver 
treatment  (Fig.  3)  had  an  epithelium  subjacent  to  the  cu- 
ticle that  was  markedly  thinner  than  that  of  the  silver 
stained  filaments  and  had  few,  if  any,  basal  and  apical 
infoldings. 

Ultrastructure  of  the  gill  filaments 

Choice  of  filaments  for  ultrastructural  analysis  of  pre- 
sumptive ion-transporting  and  respiratory  tissue  was  based 


upon  the  distribution  of  the  two  types  as  indicated  by 
silver  staining  (Fig.  1 )  and  proved  to  be  accurate  in  all  20 
of  the  filaments  examined.  No  differences  were  observed 
among  sections  from  proximal,  medial,  or  distal  regions 
of  a  filament.  Diagrammatic  representations  of  cross  sec- 
tions through  the  two  types  of  filaments  are  seen  in  Figures 
4  and  5.  The  transporting  filaments,  regardless  of  the  gill 
set,  had  a  diameter  of  0.1  mm  to  0.2  mm,  while  the  re- 
spiratory filaments  were  0.2  mm  to  0.3  mm  in  diameter. 
Both  types  of  filaments  contained  an  afferent  and  an  ef- 
ferent blood  channel  and  lateral  lacunae.  The  afferent 
channel  was  on  the  side  of  the  filament  that  faced  toward 
the  gill  stalk,  and  the  efferent  channel  on  the  side  that 


158 


J.  S.  DICKSON  ET  AL 


Figure  2.     TEM  of  transporting  gill  filament  after  silver  staining.  Note  crystals  within  and  on  the  cuticle 
(arrows).  Bar  =  5  nm. 

Figure  3.     TEM  of  respiratory  gill  filament  after  silver  staining.  Bar  =  1.0  pm. 


faced  away  from  the  stalk.  A  septum  and  the  surrounding 
connective  tissue  separated  the  two  major  blood  spaces. 
An  epithelium,  80-90%  of  which  occurred  in  the  lacunar 
blood  space,  lined  the  noncalcified  cuticle  of  the  filaments. 
Epithelial  cells  extended  across  the  lacunae  forming  pillar 
structures  with  their  basal  portion  embedded  in  loose 
connective  tissue.  Also  depicted  is  the  marked  difference 
in  epithelial  thickness  within  and  between  the  filament 
types. 

In  the  transporting  filaments,  the  cuticle  epithelium 
bordering  the  afferent  channel  was  the  thickest  (approx- 
imately 6  ^m)  (Fig.  4).  Numerous  mitochondria,  generally 
oriented  perpendicular  to  the  cuticle,  were  observed  and 
were  most  abundant  in  the  apical  portion  of  the  cells  (Figs. 
6,  7,  8).  Apical  microvillar  processes  were  approximately 
80  nm  wide  and  varied  in  length  from  0.4  ^m  to  1.2  pm 
(Fig.  8).  Electron-dense  material  was  observed  between 
the  tips  of  the  microvilli  and  the  overlying  cuticle  (Fig. 
9).  Numerous  infoldings  and  interdigitations  occurred  in 
the  basal  region  of  the  epithelium  (Figs.  6,  7).  The  micro- 
tubules  observed  within  the  cytoplasm  were  oriented 
nearly  perpendicular  to  the  cuticle  and  were  often  closely 
apposed  to  mitochondria  (Fig.  8).  Occasional  golgi  ap- 
parati  were  observed  in  the  epithelium  lining  the  outer 
margin  of  the  afferent  canal  (Fig.  6),  as  well  as  a  thin  basal 
lamina  (Figs.  6,  9).  Adhering  junctions  occurred  apically 
on  lateral  cell  membranes,  below  which  were  septate 
junctions.  Gap  junctions  were  observed  below  the  septate 
junctions  (Fig.  6). 

The  portion  of  the  epithelium  lateral  to  the  septum  and 
on  the  efferent  side  of  the  filament  was  thinner  (approx- 
imately 1.5  nm)  than  that  portion  bordering  the  afferent 
canal,  but  also  contained  numerous  mitochondria  and 
microvillar  processes  (Fig.  9). 


The  epithelial  cells  forming  the  pillar  structures  had  a 
highly  convoluted  basal  nucleus  (Fig.  7)  and  were  usually 
rich  in  mitochondria,  rough  endoplasmic  reticulum,  and 
golgi  apparati  (Fig.  8). 

The  cuticle  of  the  transporting  gill  filaments  was  of  uni- 
form thickness  (approximately  1 .8  j/m)  around  their  entire 
circumference.  An  outer  thin  epicuticle,  the  thin  lamellae 
of  the  exocuticle,  and  the  thicker  lamellae  of  the  endo- 
cuticle  were  easily  differentiated  in  most  sections  (Figs.  6, 
9).  The  approximate  thicknesses  of  the  exocuticle  and  the 
endocuticle  were  0.8  ^m  and  0.6  ^m,  respectively. 

The  septum  traversed  the  central  blood  space,  joining 
the  loose  connective  tissue  on  either  side  of  the  filament, 
thereby  forming  a  partition  between  the  afferent  and  ef- 
ferent blood  channels  (Figs.  4,  5,  10,  16).  Along  the  efferent 
channel  the  septum  was  relatively  smooth,  whereas  the 
side  facing  the  afferent  channel  possessed  processes  that 
extended  into  the  channel  (Figs.  10,  16).  In  the  trans- 
porting filaments  the  septum  was  relatively  thin  (approx- 
imately 1.1  ^m)  (Fig.  10)  and  became  shorter  as  the  fil- 
ament tapered  in  its  distal  portions.  Numerous  mem- 
branes were  observed  along  the  length  of  the  septum,  some 
of  which  constituted  the  cell  membranes  of  loose  con- 
nective tissue  cells  on  either  side  of  the  filament.  A  thin 
basal  lamina  was  observed  on  the  efferent  side  of  the  sep- 
tum, and  a  thicker  fibrous  basement  membrane  covered 
the  afferent  septal  process  (Figs.  10,  16). 

The  epithelium  of  the  respiratory  filament  varied  in 
thickness,  ranging  from  0.7  ^m  opposite  the  septum  to 
3.0  nm  in  regions  adjacent  to  pillar  structures  (Figs.  5, 
1 1 ).  A  few  scattered  round  or  oval  mitochondria  were 
observed  in  the  granular  cytoplasm  whereas  thicker  re- 
gions of  the  epithelium  adjacent  to  the  pillar  cells  also 
had  some  glycogen  granules  (Figs.  12.  13).  The  regions  of 


GILL  FILAMENTS  IN  P   CLARkll 


159 


Figures  -4  and  5.  Diagrammatic  representations  of  cross  sections 
through  a  transporting  I  Fig.  4)  and  respiratory  (Fig.  5)  gill  filament.  Note 
the  afferent  blood  channel  (Ab).  efferent  blood  channel  (Eb),  epithelium 
(E),  cuticle  (c),  septum  (s).  podocyte  (P),  hemocyte  (h).  pillar  structure 
(p),  and  lacuna  (1). 


the  epithelium  lateral  to  the  septum  and  on  the  efferent 
side  of  the  filament  were  much  thinner,  having  a  width 
as  small  as  0.08  ^m  (Figs.  11,  14).  The  epithelium  was 
lined  by  a  thin  basal  lamina  and  had  a  sparse  cytoplasm 
containing  microtubules.  Fibrous  material  was  also  seen 
between  the  basal  lamina  and  the  epithelium.  No  apical 
microvillar  processes  or  basal  infoldings  were  observed  in 
the  respiratory  filament  epithelia. 

The  most  prominent  feature  of  the  cells  in  the  pillar 
regions  was  the  bundles  of  microtubules  observed  in  their 
cytoplasm  (Figs.  12,  13).  These  bundles  were  generally 
oriented  perpendicular  to  the  cuticle,  apparently  attached 


to  extensions  of  the  cuticle  that  penetrated  into  the  epi- 
thelial cells  (Fig.  13).  The  pillar  cytoplasm  contained  some 
mitochondria,  rough  endoplasmic  reticulum,  and  golgi, 
but  noticeably  fewer  than  in  the  transporting  filament  tis- 
sues (Fig.  12). 

The  cuticle  of  the  respiratory  filaments  was  thickest  on 
the  side  of  the  afferent  channel  (approximately  2.0  ^m) 
(Fig.  14)  and  thinnest  on  the  efferent  side  of  the  filament 
(approximately  0.5  fim)  (Fig.  15).  The  attenuation  of  the 
cuticle  from  the  afferent  to  the  efferent  side  was  observed 
lateral  to  the  septum  of  the  filament.  It  appeared  as  if  no 
endocuticle  had  been  deposited  on  the  afferent  side  of  the 
filament,  the  side  facing  the  carapace. 

The  septum  of  the  respiratory  filament  was  thicker  than 
the  transporting  filament  septum,  possessing  afferent  sep- 
tal  projections,  a  thick  afferent  basement  membrane,  and 
a  thin  efferent  basal  lamina.  The  septum  had  a  width  of 
about  5  ^m,  with  an  abundance  of  cell  membrane  inter- 
digitations  (Figs.  5,  16).  A  thick  layer  of  fibrous  material 
frequently  lined  the  inside  of  the  cell  membrane  on  the 
afferent  side  of  the  septum  (Fig.  16).  Thicker  septa  ob- 
served in  these  filaments  usually  had  loose  connective 
tissue  extending  across  the  septum  on  the  afferent  side 
(Fig.  5). 

Neural  tissue  was  often  observed  in  both  filament  types 
(Fig.  17).  The  small  nerves  were  located  within  one  of  the 
small  blood  spaces  of  the  connective  tissue  lateral  to  the 
septum.  Usually  there  was  one  nerve  per  filament,  though 
a  few  filaments  were  observed  that  contained  two  or  three 
nerves,  each  in  a  separate  blood  space.  The  nerve  was 
continuous  throughout  the  length  of  a  filament.  Nerves 
ranged  from  1 .0  ^m  to  2.7  jum  in  diameter  and  contained 
two  to  six  neurons.  The  neurons,  which  ranged  from  0. 16 
fim  to  1.5  Mm  in  diameter,  contained  numerous,  evenly 
spaced  neurotubules.  No  cell  bodies,  neurosecretory 
granules,  or  synapses  were  observed  in  any  section. 

Also  occurring  in  both  types  of  filaments  were  cells 
resembling  "podocytes"  (Morse  et  ai,  1970).  They  were 
observed  near  the  septum,  attached  to  the  loose  connective 
tissue  bordering  the  afferent  channel  (Figs.  4,  5,  18)  and 
were  surrounded  by  a  basal  lamina.  These  cells  possessed 
an  interdigitating  cell  membrane,  or  pedicels,  which 
formed  extracellular  spaces  between  the  pedicels  and  the 
cell  body.  Much  of  the  cytoplasm  contained  an  abundance 
of  smooth  endoplasmic  reticulum.  Coated  pits  and  vesicles 
were  often  observed  along  or  near  the  cell  membrane  and 
numerous  dense  granules,  varying  in  size  and  number, 
occurred  within  the  cytoplasm  along  with  golgi  apparati 
consisting  of  thin,  curved  cisternae  (Fig.  19). 

A  TPase  determinations 

As  shown  in  Figure  20,  the  mean  for  total  ATPase  ac- 
tivity was  higher  in  the  transporting  than  in  the  respiratory 


160 


J.  S.  DICKSON  ET  AL 


Figures  6-9.  TEM  ot'lransporting  filaments.  Figure  6.  Section  showing  the  cuticle  with  its  three  layers, 
the  epicuticle  (arrow),  the  exocuticle  (Ex)  and  the  endocuticle  (En).  Also  note  a  Golgi  apparatus  (g).  the 
basal  lamina  (b).  and  a  junctional  complex  (arrowhead).  Bar  =  1.0  pm.  Figure  7.  Section  showing  a  pillar 
structure  (p)  separating  two  lacunar  spaces  (1)  from  an  efferent  blood  channel  (Eb).  Note  heterochromatic 
nucleus  (n).  Bar  =  5.0  ^m.  Figure  8.  Section  of  epithelium  under  cuticle  showing  microtubules  (arrowheads) 
and  rough  endoplasmic  reticulum  (r)  in  the  cytoplasm.  Also  note  the  apical  microvillar  structures  (arrow) 
immediately  below  the  cuticle.  Bar  =  1 .0  ^m.  Figure  9.  Section  of  epithelium  between  cuticle  and  lacuna 
showing  the  electron  dense  material  at  the  tip  of  the  microvillar  processes  (arrowhead)  and  the  thin  basal 
lamina  (b).  Bar  =  1.0  pm. 


GILL  FILAMENTS  IN  P   CLIRK1I 


161 


Ab 


Figures  10-16.  TEM  of  transporting  (10)  and  respiratory  (11-16)  filaments.  Figure  10.  Section  of  a 
septum  in  a  transporting  filament  that  separates  the  afferent  (Ah)  from  the  efferent  (Eh)  hlood  channel.  Bar 
=  2.0^11.  Figure  11.  Section  of  a  respiratory  filament  showing  the  thin  epithelium  underlying  the  cuticle. 
Also  note  the  pillar  structure  (p)  and  a  hemocyte  (h).  Bar  =  5.0  ^m.  Figure  12.  Pillar  structure  of  a 
respiratory  filament  showing  the  bundles  of  microtubules  running  perpendicular  to  the  cuticle  (arrowheads). 
Bar  =  1.0  nim.  Figure  13.  Section  showing  relationship  of  microtubules  (m)  to  extensions  of  the  cuticle 
(arrowheads).  Bar  =  0.2  /jm.  Figure  14.  Cuticle  on  side  of  afferent  channel.  Bar  =  I.O/im.  Figure  15.  Cuticle 
on  side  of  efferent  channel.  Bar  =  1.0  ^m.  Figure  16.  Septum  from  respiratory  filament  separating  the 
afferent  blood  channel  (Ah)  from  the  efferent  blood  channel  (Eb).  Note  the  thin  basal  lamina  on  the  efferent 
side  (arrowhead)  as  compared  with  the  thick  layer  on  the  afferent  side  (arrow).  Bar  =  1.0  /im. 


162 


J.  S.  DICKSON  AT  AL 


Figures  17-19.  TEM  of  filaments.  Figure  17.  Cross  section  of  nerve  showing  individual  neurons  (n) 
having  evenly  spaced  neurotuhules  (arrowheads).  Bar  =  0.5  /jm.  Figure  18.  Podocyte  (P)  in  loose  connective 
tissue  (Ct).  Note  electron-dense  inclusions  (d).  Bar  =  5.0  ^m.  Figure  19.  Podocyte  cytoplasm  showing 
pedicels  (arrowheads)  golgi  (g)  and  basal  lamina  (arrow).  Bar  =  1.0  ^m. 


filaments;  however,  the  difference  between  the  means  was 
not  significant.  Mean  values  for  Mg-ATPase  were  very 
similar  between  respiratory  and  transporting  filaments  and 
were  likewise  not  significantly  different.  The  means  for 
Na,  K-ATPase,  in  contrast,  were  significantly  different  (P 
<  0.001,  Mann-Whitney  U  test),  with  the  transporting 
filaments  having  more  than  five  times  as  much  mean  ac- 
tivity as  the  respiratory  filaments. 

Transepithelial  potentials 

The  values  of  the  TEP's  in  the  filaments  were  indepen- 
dent of  the  region  (proximal,  medial,  or  distal)  of  the  fil- 


ament impaled  and  of  the  podobranch  upon  which  the 
filament  was  located.  The  TEP's  of  filaments  that  were 
presumed  to  be  respiratory  (based  upon  the  silver  staining 
results)  ranged  from  -6  to  -18  mV  (-1 1.9  ±  3.2  mV, 
mean  ±  S.D.,  n  =  19).  The  TEP's  measured  in  presump- 
tive transport  filaments  ranged  from  -21  to  -36  mV 
(-28.1  ±  4.7  mV,  mean  ±  S.D.;  n  =  21).  There  was  no 
overlap  in  the  frequency  distributions  of  the  ranges  of 
TEP's  (Fig.  2 1 )  and  the  difference  between  the  means  was 
highly  significant  (P  <  0.00 1 ,  two-tailed  t  test).  The  sodium 
concentration  of  the  medium  was  51  mA/and  that  of  the 
hemolymph  was  107.5  ±  15.0  mA/  (mean  ±  S.D., 
n  =  11). 


GILL  FILAMENTS  IN  P.  CLARKII 


163 


/^.rnol    P 
mg    protein*hr 


Total 


Na.K 


Figure  20.  Mean  (±  standard  deviation)  ATPase  activity  comparing 
pooled  transporting  (open  bars)  and  respiratory  (solid  bars)  filaments. 
**  =  P<  0.001 


Discussion 

The  results  of  silver  staining,  ultrastructural  analyses, 
ATPase  measurements,  and  measurements  of  TEP  all  in- 
dicate that  the  gills  of  P.  clarkii  are  not  homogeneous  in 
structure,  but  contain  filaments  dedicated  to  ion  transport 
and  filaments  dedicated  to  respiration.  No  filaments  had 
both  characteristics;  rather  individual  filaments  were 
uniquely  respiratory  or  transporting.  Respiratory  filaments 
occurred  on  the  lateral  rows  of  most  gills,  whereas  the 
transporting  filaments  predominated  in  the  central  bed 
of  the  gills.  This  distribution  of  the  two  filament  types  in 
discrete  regions  of  the  gill  placed  the  respiratory  filaments 
in  the  path  of  the  most  rapid  water  flow  (Burggren  el  ai, 
1974). 

The  respiratory  filaments  had  a  very  thin,  squamous 
epithelium,  whereas  the  ion  transporting  filaments  pos- 
sessed a  markedly  thicker  epithelium.  The  ultrastructure 
of  the  ion  transporting  epithelia  observed  in  P.  clarkii  gill 
filaments  was  similar  to  ion  transporting  epithelia  de- 
scribed in  other  crayfish  (Morse  el  a/.,  1970;  Bielawski. 
1971;  Fisher,  1972),  in  isopods  (Bubel  and  Jones,  1974), 
in  crabs  (Copeland  and  Fitzjarrell,  1968;  Taylor  and 
Greenaway,  1979;  Finol  and  Croghan,  1983),  in  shrimp 
(Foster  and  Howse,  1978),  and  in  Dapluua  (Kikuchi, 
1983).  Apical  microvillar  processes  were  present  to  in- 
crease the  surface  area  over  which  ions  could  be  trans- 
ported as  well  as  numerous  basal  infoldings  with  associated 
mitochondria.  Such  basal  infoldings  are  reported  to  be 
the  major  site  of  Na,  K-ATPase  (Diamond  and  Bossert, 
1968;  Ernst,  1972;  Ernst  et  ai.  1981;  Towle,  1985;Towle 
and  Kays,  1986).  This  would  be  consistent  with  the  higher 
Na,  K-ATPase  activity  measured  in  transporting  fila- 
ments, because  few  basal  infoldings  or  mitochondria  were 
observed  in  the  epithelia  of  the  respiratory  filaments. 


The  structure  of  the  thin  squamous  epithelia  in  P.  clar- 
kii respiratory  filaments  was  similar  to  the  respiratory  ep- 
ithelia described  in  other  crayfish  (Dunel-Erb  et  ai,  1982) 
in  crabs  (Copeland  and  Fitzjarrell,  1968;  Taylor  and 
Greenaway.  1979),  and  in  shrimp  (Foster  and  Howse, 
1978).  The  paucity  of  organelles  observed  in  the  epithe- 
lium and  pillar  structures  indicates  that  little  cellular  ac- 
tivity occurs  in  these  cells  other  than  maintenance  of  cell 
components.  The  thin  epithelial  layer  may  serve  as  a  per- 
meability barrier  to  the  diffusive  loss  of  ions  and  blood 
proteins,  while  the  thin  cytoplasm  would  allow  efficient 
gas  exchange  to  occur. 

Large  bundles  of  microtubules  were  observed  in  the 
pillar  structures  of  the  respiratory  filaments,  whereas  single 
microtubules  were  the  rule  in  the  transporting  epithelia 
of  this  crayfish  and  in  most  gill  epithelia  of  other  crusta- 
ceans (Copeland  and  Fitzjarrell,  1968;  Bielawski,  1971; 
Foster  and  Howse,  1978;  Taylor  and  Greenaway,  1979; 
Finol  and  Croghan,  1983;  Compere  et  ai,  1989).  These 
microtubules  appear  to  anchor  the  epithelium  to  the  cu- 
ticle. Finol  and  Croghan  (1983)  have  proposed  that  the 
microtubules  function  to  stabilize  the  gill  epithelium 
against  the  hydrostatic  pressure  of  the  blood.  Because  the 
respiratory  epithelium  contains  very  little  cytoplasm,  ad- 
ditional microtubules,  in  the  form  of  bundles  near  the 
periphery  of  the  pillar  structures,  may  give  the  additional 
support  needed  to  withstand  the  shear  forces  of  the  blood 
flow  in  these  filaments. 

The  loose  connective  tissue  in  the  gills  of  P.  clarkii  is 
similar  to  that  observed  in  Callinectes  sapidus  (Johnson, 
1980).  The  cells  within  the  loose  connective  tissue,  with 
their  abundant  glycogen  rosettes  and  granules,  may  reg- 
ulate the  blood  glucose  levels  as  suggested  by  Finol  and 
Croghan  ( 1983)  for  Uca  more/a. 


Frequency 


-16  -24 

T.E.P    (mV) 


-32 


-40 


Figure  21.     Transepithelial  potentials  (TEP)  for  respiratory  (solid  bars, 
n  =  19)  and  transporting  (open  bars,  n  =  21)  filaments. 


164 


J.  S.  DICKSON 


The  observed  difference  in  the  size  and  structure  of  the 
septum  in  the  two  types  of  filaments  suggests  its  function 
may  likewise  differ.  The  thicker  septum  in  the  respiratory 
filaments  may  make  the  vascular  canals  more  rigid,  or  it 
may  also  be  a  more  substantial  barrier  to  diffusion  between 
the  two  compartments. 

The  neurons  observed  in  the  filaments  of  P.  clarkii  were 
similar  to  those  constituting  the  branchial  nerve  observed 
in  A.  pallipes  and  A.  leptodactylus(Dune\-Erb  el  al,  1 982). 
While  those  authors  have  described  the  nerve  cell  bodies 
and  described  structures  resembling  neurosecretory  gran- 
ules, the  role  of  the  nerve  is  uncertain.  Massabuau  el  al. 
( 1 980)  and  Ishii  el  al.  ( 1 989)  have  suggested  that  the  nerve 
may  serve  peripheral  sensory  elements  involved  in  oxygen 
sensing.  While  the  presence  of  the  nerve  in  both  the  re- 
spiratory and  transporting  filaments  would  seem  to  ques- 
tion this  function,  the  absence  of  any  description  of  a 
transducing  element  leaves  this  question  unresolved. 

The  "podocytes"  observed  in  the  filament  tissues  pos- 
sessed the  same  ultrastructure  as  "podocytes"  observed 
in  other  crustacean  gills  (Wright,  1964;  Strangeways- 
Dixon  and  Smith,  1970:  Doughtie  and  Rao,  1981).  The 
presence  of  coated  vesicles  and  large  storage  vacuoles  gives 
support  to  the  hypothesis  that  these  cells  take  up  toxic 
substances  and  blood  components  from  the  hemolymph 
for  degradation  or  storage  (Strangeways-Dixon  and  Smith, 
1970;  Doughtie  and  Rao,  1981). 

ATPase  measurements  supported  the  silver  staining  and 
morphological  observations  in  that  values  were  highest  in 
the  transporting  filaments.  These  data  represent  an  ap- 
parent contradiction  to  the  conclusion  of  Wheatly  and 
Henry  (1987)  who  reported  that  enzyme  activity  was  ho- 
mogeneously distributed  throughout  the  branchial  tissue. 
However,  Wheatly  and  Henry  (1987)  pooled  tissue  from 
entire  gill  sets,  and,  therefore,  could  not  have  detected 
differences  at  the  level  of  individual  filaments.  In  fact, 
their  data  would  represent  a  mean  of  respiratory  and 
transporting  filaments,  assuming  that  Pacifastacm  has  a 
distribution  of  filament  types  similar  to  Procambarus.  Our 
values  for  Na,  K-ATPase  activity  in  respiratory  filaments 
are  comparable  to  theirs,  but  the  values  we  obtained  for 
transport  filaments  are  fivefold  higher,  reflecting  the  en- 
richment of  transport  tissue  resulting  from  analyses  of 
selected  filaments. 

While  the  in  situ  measurements  of  TEP's  in  the  gill 
filaments  were  not  intended  to  supply  extensive  data  on 
the  mechanisms  of  transport,  a  few  interesting  conclusions 
may  be  made.  The  data  clearly  suggest  two  functionally 
different  populations  of  filaments.  These  data  are  quali- 
tatively similar  to  those  of Pequeux  and  Gilles  (1988)  who 
found  positive  TEP's  in  isolated,  anterior,  respiratory  gills 
of  Eriocheir,  but  negative  potentials  in  the  posterior, 
transporting  gills. 


The  fact  that  the  TEP's  were  negative  with  respect  to 
the  dilute  medium,  suggests  that  under  the  experimental 
circumstances  the  integumental  barrier  is  preferentially 
cation  conductive.  The  TEP's  of  the  respiratory  filaments 
were  slightly  more  positive  and  those  of  the  transport  fil- 
aments slightly  more  negative  than  the  diffusion  potential 
for  Na  (-19  mV)  calculated  from  the  measured  concen- 
tration difference.  Cation  selectivity  is  consistent  with  the 
data  from  other  gill  potential  studies  (Austropotamobius 
-  Astacus  pallipes,  Croghan  et  al.,  1965;  Callinectes, 
Mantel,  1967)  and  a  study  on  the  isolated  gill  cuticle  of 
Carcinus  (Lignon,  1987).  Only  a  study  by  Avenet  and 
Lignon  (1985)  presented  data  showing  anion  selectivity 
in  the  isolated  cuticle  from  the  gill  lamina  (plate)  of  As- 
tacus  leptodactylus.  Whether  the  TEP's  arise  from  diffu- 
sion potentials  or  from  electrogenic  transport  is  unknown, 
however,  thecontributionoftransport-generated  potentials 
is  generally  small  in  gills  in  the  presence  (Siebers  et  al.. 
1985)  or  in  the  absence  of  concentration  gradients  (Siebers 
et  al..  1985;  Drews  and  Graszynski,  1987).  Avenet  and 
Lignon  (1985)  and  Lignon  (1987)  suggested  that  the  ion 
selectivity  of  the  integumental  barrier  resided  in  the  epi- 
cuticle.  Such  cation  selectivity  of  the  epicuticle  of  the  gill 
of  Procambarus  would  be  consistent  with  the  location  of 
the  silver  precipitate  within  the  exocuticle.  Presumably, 
the  divalent  silver  cations  could  easily  cross  the  outer  cu- 
ticular  barrier  to  precipitate  with  higher  internal  concen- 
trations of  chloride  or,  more  likely,  bicarbonate.  If  the 
observed  TEP's  are  the  result  of  diffusion  potentials,  the 
differences  in  TEP's  between  the  respiratory  and  transport 
filaments  would  suggest  either  different  ion  permeabilities 
or  different  local  ion  concentrations  within  the  two  types 
of  filaments. 

The  circulation  of  hemolymph  within  the  gill  filament 
has  been  described  by  Bock  (1925),  Fisher  (1972),  and 
Burggren  et  al.  (1974).  They  describe  hemolymph  as 
flowing  toward  the  tip  of  the  filament  in  the  afferent  chan- 
nel and  down  the  filament  toward  the  gill  stalk  in  the 
efferent  channel.  Hemolymph  may  also  be  shunted  from 
the  afferent  to  the  efferent  channels  via  the  lateral  lacunae. 
The  thick  septum  of  connective  tissue  in  respiratory  fil- 
aments may  act  as  a  permeability  barrier  to  counter-cur- 
rent gas  exchange  between  the  deoxygenated  afferent  he- 
molymph and  the  oxygenated  efferent  hemolymph,  be- 
cause this  type  of  exchange  would  result  in  deoxygenated 
blood  being  returned  to  the  body.  The  thin  septum  of  the 
transporting  filaments,  on  the  other  hand,  may  promote 
counter-current  diffusion  of  ions  from  the  afferent  to  the 
efferent  channel,  preventing  further  diffusive  ion  loss  from 
the  lacunae. 

The  observations  in  the  present  study  indicate  that  there 
is  a  precise  partitioning  of  structural  and  functional  epi- 
thelium types  within  the  gills  of  Procambarus  clarkii,  a 
thin  squamous  epithelium  functioning  in  respiration  and 


GILL  FILAMENTS  IN  P   CLARKI1 


165 


a  thick,  mitochondria-rich  epithelium  functioning  in  ion 
transport.  It  is  important  to  remember,  however,  that  the 
integument  consists  not  only  of  the  underlying  epithelium, 
but  also  of  the  overlying  cuticle.  Because  this  cuticle  must 
periodically  be  shed  to  permit  growth,  the  question  arises 
as  to  whether  the  specialized  functions  of  the  filaments 
are  disrupted  during  the  period  surrounding  ecdysis.  Dur- 
ing premolt  the  hypodermis  is  normally  engaged  in  cu- 
ticular  synthesis,  and  prior  to  ecdysis  the  preexuvial  cuticle 
exists  as  an  additional  barrier  to  the  movement  of  sub- 
stances. A  more  complete  understanding  of  gill  function 
must  await  examination  of  gills  not  only  during  intermolt, 
but  also  during  the  molt. 

Literature  Cited 

Avenet,  P.,  and  J.  M.  Lignon.  1985.  Ionic  permeabilities  of  the  gill 
lamina  of  the  crayfish.  Astacus  leptodacly/us  (E).  ./  Physiol.  363: 
377-401. 

Bielawski,  J.  1971.     Ultrastructure  and  ion  transport  in  gill  epithelium 
of  the  crayfish,  Astacus  leplodaclylus  Esch.  Protuplasma  73:  177 
190. 

Bock,  F.  1925.     Die  Respirationsorgane  von  Potamolmis  astacin,  Leach. 
(Astacus  flimatilis  Fabr.)  Ein  Beitrag  zur  Morphologic  der  Decapoden. 
Z.  H'iss.  Zool.  124:  51-117. 
Bryan,  G.  \V.  1959.     Sodium  regulation  in  the  crayfish  Astacusfluviatilis. 

I.  The  normal  animal.  J.  E.\p.  Biol.  37:  83-99. 

Bubel,  A.,  and  M.  B.  Jones.  1974.  Fine  structure  of  the  gills  of  Jacra 
«o«/wuwi/(Rathke)  [Crustacea,  Isopoda].  J.  Mar  Biol  Assoc  L'  K. 
54:  737-743. 

Burggren,     W.     VV.,     B.     R.     McMahon.    and    J.     \V.    Costerton. 

1974.     Branchial  water-  and  blood-flow  patterns  and  the  structure 

of  the  gill  of  the  crayfish  Pmcambariis  clarkit.  Can-  J.  tool.  52:  1511- 

1518. 

Cioffi,  M.  1984.     Comparative  ultrastructure  of  arthropod  transporting 

epithelia.  Am.  Zool.  24:  139-156. 

Compere,  P.,  S.  Wanson,  A.  Pequeux,  R.  Gilles,  and  G.  Goffmet. 

1989.     Ultrastructural  changes  in  the  gill  epithelium  of  the  green 

crab  Cardmts  maenas  in  relation  to  the  external  salinity.  Tissue  Cell 

21:299-318. 

Copeland,  D.  E.  1963.     Possible  osmoregulatory  cells  in  crab  gills.  J 

Cell  Biol.  19:  16A. 
Copeland,  D.  E.  1967.     A  study  of  salt  secreting  cells  in  the  bnne  shrimp 

(Anemia  sa/ina).  Protoplasma  63:  363-384. 
Copeland,  D.  E.  1968.     Fine  structure  of  salt  and  water  uptake  in  the 

land-crab,  (Jecarcmus  lateralis.  Am.  Zool.  8:  417-432. 
Copeland,  D.  E.,  and  A.  T.  Fitzjarrell.  1968.     The  salt  absorbing  cells 
in  the  gills  of  the  blue  crab  (Callinectes  sapiiiiis  Rathbun)  with  notes 
on  modified  mitochondria.  Z.  Zelljorscli.  92:  1-22. 
Croghan,  P.  C,  R.  A.  Curra,  and  A.  P.  M.  Lockwood.  1965.     The  elec- 
trical potential  difference  across  the  isolated  gills  of  the  crayfish  Aus- 
tropotamobiiis  pallipes  (Lereboullet).  /  Exp.  Biol.  42:  463-474. 
Diamond,  J.  M.,  and  W.  H.  Bossert.  1968.     Functional  consequences 
of  ultrastructural  geometry  in  "backwards"  fluid-transporting  epi- 
thelia. J.  Cell  Bio/  37:  694-702. 

Doughtie,  D.  G.  and  K.  R.  Rao.  1981.  The  syncytial  nature  and  phago- 
cytic  activity  of  the  branchial  podocytes  in  the  grass  shrimp,  Palae- 
monetes  pugiu.  Tissue  Cell  13:  93-104. 

Drach,  P.,  and  C.  Tchernigovtzeff.  1967.  Sur  la  methode  de  determi- 
nation des  stades  d'intermue  et  son  application  generate  aux  crustaces. 
Vie  Milieu  18:  595-610. 


Drews,  G.,  and  K.  Graszynski.  1987.  The  transepithelial  potential  dif- 
ference in  the  gills  of  the  fiddler  crab,  L'ca  lanxcri  influence  of  some 
inhibitors.  J.  Comp  Physiol  157B:  345-353. 

Dunel-Erb,  S.,  J.-C.  Massabuau,  and  P.  C.  Laurent.  1982.     Organisation 
fonctionelle  de  la  branchie  d'ecrevisse.  C  R  Seances  Soc  Biol.  176: 
248-258. 
F.hrenfeld,  J.  1974.     Aspects  of  ionic  transport  mechanisms  in  crayfish 

Abacus  leptodacly/us  J.  Exp.  Biol.  61:  57-70. 

Ernst,  S.  A.  1972.  Transport  adenosine  triphosphatase  cytochemistry. 
II.  Cytochemical  localization  of  ouabain-sensitive.  potassium-depen- 
dent phosphatase  activity  in  the  secretory  epithelium  of  the  avian 
salt  gland.  J.  Histoehem.  Cytnehem.  20:  23-28. 

Ernst,  S.  A.,  C.  V.  Riddle,  and  K.  J.  Karnaky,  Jr.  1981.  Relationship 
between  localization  of  Na+-K+-ATPase,  cellular  fine  structure,  and 
reabsorptive  and  secretory  electrolyte  transport.  Pp.  355-385  in  Cur- 
rent Topics  in  Membranes  and  Transport.  V.  30.  F.  Bronner  and  A. 
Kleinzeller.  eds.  Academic  Press.  New  York. 

Finol,  H.  J.,  and  P.  C.  Croghan.  1983.  Ultrastructure  of  the  branchial 
epithelium  of  an  amphibious  brackish-water  crab.  Tissue  Cell  15: 
63-75. 

Fisher,  J.  M.  1972.     Fine-structural  observations  on  the  gill  filaments 
of  the  freshwater  crayfish.  Astacus  pallipes  Lereboullet.  Tissue  Cell 
4:  287-299. 
Foster,  C.  A.,  and  H.  D.  Howse.  1978.     A  morphological  study  on  gills 

of  the  brown  shrimp,  Penaeus  a:tecus.  Tissue  Cell  10:  77-92. 
Holliday,  C.  \V.  1985.     Salinity-induced  changes  in  gill  Na,  K-ATPase 
activity  in  the  mud  fiddler  crab,  Ucapugnax.  J  Exp.  Zool.  233:  199- 
208. 

Horiuchi,  S.  1977.  Characterization  of  gill  Na,  K-ATPase  in  the  fresh- 
water crayfish,  Procambarus  clarkii  (Girard).  Comp  Biochem.  Phys- 
nil.  56B:  135-138. 

Huxley,  T.  H.  1896.  The  Crayfish— An  Introduction  to  the  Study  of 
Zoo/ogv  The  Internal.  Scientific  Series.  Paul,  Trench  and  Trubner. 
London. 

Johnson,  P.  T.  1 980.     The  III  stology  of  the  Blue  Crab.  Callinectes  sap- 
idus. A  Model  for  the  Decapoda.  Praeger.  New  York.  Pp.  34-40. 
Ishii,  K.,  K.  Ishii,  J.-C.  Massabuau,  and  P.  Dejours.  1989.     Oxygen- 
sensitive  chemoreceptors  in  the  branchio-cardiac  veins  of  the  crayfish, 
Astacus  leptodactylns.  Resp.  Physiol.  78:73-81. 

Kikuchi,  S.  1983.     The  fine  structure  of  the  gill  epithelium  of  a  fresh- 
water flea.  Daphma  magna  (Crustacea:  Phyllopoda)  and  changes  as- 
sociated with  acclimation  to  various  salinities.  I.  Normal  fine  structure. 
Cell  Tissue  Rex  229:  253-268. 
Lignon,  J.  M.  1987.     Ionic  permeabilities  of  the  isolated  gill  cuticle  of 

the  shore  crab  Curcinus  maenas.  J  Exp.  Biol  131:  159-174. 
Maluf,  N.  S.  R.  1940.     The  uptake  of  inorganic  electrolytes  by  the  cray- 
fish. J.  Gen  Physiol.  24:  151-167. 

Mantel,  L.  H.  1967.  Asymmetry  potentials,  metabolism  and  sodium 
fluxes  in  gills  of  the  blue  crab,  Callinectes  sapidus.  Comp.  Biochem. 
Physiol.  20:  743-753. 

Mantel,  L.  H.,  and  J.  Landesman.  1977.  Osmotic  regulation  and  Na- 
K.-activated  ATPase  in  the  green  crab.  Carcinus  maenas  and  the 
spider  crab  Lihinia  emarginata.  Biol.  Bull.  153:  437-438. 
Massabuau,  J.-C.,  B.  Elancher,  and  P.  Dejours.  1980.  Ventilatory  reflex 
response  to  hypoxia  in  the  crayfish,  Astacus  pallipes.  J.  Comp  Physiol. 
140:  193-198. 

Milne,  D.  J.,  and  R.  A.  Ellis.  1973.  The  effect  of  salinity  acclimation 
on  the  ultrastructure  of  the  gills  ofGammarus  oceamcus  (Segerstrale. 
1947)  (Crustacea:  Amphipoda).  Z.  Zelljorscli.  139:  311-318. 
Morse,  H.  C.,  P.  J.  Harris,  and  E.  J.  Dornheld.  1970.  Pacifastacus 
leniusculus:  fine  structure  of  the  arthrobranch  with  reference  to  active 
ion  uptake.  Trans.  Am.  Micr.  Soc.  89:  12-27. 

Neufeld,  G.  J.,  C.  W.  Holliday,  and  J.  B.  Pritchard.  1980.  Salinity 
adaptation  of  gill  Na.  K-ATPase  in  the  blue  crab,  Callinectes  sapidus. 
J.  Exp.  Zool.  211:  215-224. 


166 


J.  S.  DICK.SON  ET  AL 


Pequeux,  A.,  and  R.  Gilles.  1978.  Osmoregulation  of  the  euryhaline 
Chinese  crab  Enochen  xiiienstx.  Ionic  transports  across  isolated  per- 
fused gills  as  related  to  the  salinity  of  the  environment.  Pp.  105-1  1 1 
in  Pnif.  I2lh  EMBS,  Stirling,  Scotland.  Pergamon  Press.  Oxford. 

Pequeux,  A.,  and  R.  Gilles.  1981.  Na*  fluxes  across  isolated  perfused 
gills  of  the  Chinese  crab  Eriocheir  smensis.  J  Exp.  Biot  92:  173- 
186. 

Pequeux,  A.,  and  R.  Gilles.  1988.  The  transepithelial  potential  difference 
of  isolated  perfused  gills  of  the  Chinese  crab  Eriocheir  sinensis  ac- 
climated to  fresh  water.  Comp.  Biochem.  Physiol.  89A:  163-172. 

Peterson,  G.  L.  1977.  A  simplification  of  the  protein  assay  method  of 
Lowry  el  a/,  which  is  more  generally  applicable.  Anal.  Biocliem.  83: 
346-356. 

Reynolds,  E.  S.  1963.  The  use  of  lead  citrate  at  high  pH  as  an  electron 
opaque  stain  in  electron  microscopy.  J  Cell  Biol  17:  208-212. 

Roer,  R.  D.,  and  M.  G.  Shelton.  1982.  The  effects  of  hydrostatic  pressure 
on  sodium  transport  in  the  crayfish,  Procamharnx  clarkii.  Coin/' 
Biochem.  Physiol.  71A:  271-276. 

Shaw,  J.  1960a.  The  absorption  of  sodium  ions  by  the  crayfish.  Asiiicif, 
pallipes  Lereboullet.  11.  The  effect  of  the  external  anion.  J.  Exp.  Biol. 
37:  534-547. 

Shaw,  J.  I960b.  The  absorption  of  sodium  ions  by  the  crayfish  Astaais 
pallipes  Lereboullet.  111.  The  effect  of  other  cations  in  the  external 
solution.  J.  Exp  Biol  37:  548-556. 

Siebers,  D.,  A.  \Vinkler,  C.  Lucu,  G.  Thedens  and  D.  VVeichart. 
1985.  Na-K-ATPase  generates  an  active  transport  potential  in  the 
gills  of  the  hyperregulating  shore  crab  Carcinus  maenas.  Mar.  Biol. 
87:  185-142. 

Spurr,  A.  R.  1969.  A  low-viscosity  epoxy  resin  embedding  medium 
for  electron  microscopy.  /  Ultraxlnicl.  Res  26:  31-43. 


Stevenson,.!.  R.  1972.  Changing  activities  of  the  crustacean  epidermis 
during  the  molt  cycle.  Am.  Znol.  12:  373-380. 

Strangeways-Dixon,  J.,  and  D.  S.  Smith.  1970.  The  fine  structure  of 
the  gill  "podocytes"  in  Panulirux  argtis  (Crustacea).  Tissue  Cell  2: 
61  1-624. 

Taylor,  H.  H.,  and  P.  Greenaway.  1979.  The  structure  of  the  gills  and 
lungs  of  the  arid-zone  crab,  Holthuixana  (Auslrolhelphusa)  Iransversa 
(Brachyura:  Sundathelphusidae)  including  observation  on  arterial 
vessels  within  the  gills.  J  /.mil.  189:  359-384. 

Towle,  D.  \V.  1984.  Membrane-bound  ATPases  in  arthropod  ion- 
transporting  tissues.  Am  /no/  24:  177-185. 

Towle,  D.  VV.  1985.  Localization  of  Na+  +  K+-ATPase  in  basolateral 
membranes  of  crab  (Carcinus  maenas)  gill  epithelium.  Bull  MD1BL 
25:  80-83. 

Towle,  D.  VV.,  and  VV.  T.  Kays.  1986.  Basolateral  localization  of  Na* 
+  K*-ATPase  in  gill  epithelium  of  two  osmoregulating  crabs.  Cal- 
linectcs  sapidus  and  Carcinus  maenas.  J.  Exp.  Zool.  239:  311-318. 

Wanson,  S.  A.,  A.  J.  R.  Pequeux,  and  R.  D.  Roer.  1984.  Na+  regulation 
and  the  (Na+  +  K+)ATPase  activity  in  the  euryhaline  fiddler  crab 
Vca  mmax  (Le  Conte).  Comp.  Biochem  Physiol.  79A:  673-678. 

VVheatly,  M.  G.,  and  R.  P.  Henry.  1987.  Branchial  and  antennal  gland 
Na+/K+-dependent  ATPase  and  carbonic  anhydrase  activity  during 
salinity  acclimation  of  the  euryhaline  crayfish  Pacifastacus  /ennixcii/us. 
J  Exp.  Biol  133:  73-86. 

Wheeler,  A.  P.  1975.  Oyster  mantle  carbonic  anhydrase:  evidence  for 
plasma  membrane  bound  activity  and  for  a  role  in  bicarbonate  trans- 
port. Ph.D.  Dissertation,  Duke  LIniversity.  Durham.  North  Carolina. 

Wright.  K.  A.  1964.  The  fine  structure  of  the  nephrocyte  of  the  gills 
of  two  marine  decapods.  J  I'llraslructiire  Res  10:  1-13. 


Reference:  Biol  Bull  180:  167-184.  (February,  1991) 


Nutrient  Translocation  during  Early  Disc  Regeneration 

in  the  Brittlestar  Microphiopholis  gracillima 

(Stimpson)  (Echinodermata:  Ophiuroidea) 


WILLIAM  E.  DOBSON1*,  STEPHEN  E.  STANCYK,  LEE  ANN  CLEMENTS2, 
AND  RICHARD  M.  SHOWMAN 

Department  of  Biology,  University  of  South  Carolina,  Columbia,  South  Carolina,  29208; 

]  Department  of  Biology.  Cla/lin  College.  Orangeburg,  South  Carolina  291 15;  and -Division  of 

Science  and  Mathematics,  Jacksonville  University.  Jacksonville,  Florida  3221 1 


Abstract.  Microphiopholis  gracillima  can  autotomize 
and  then  regenerate  the  autotomized  central  disc,  includ- 
ing integument,  gut,  and  gonads.  Experiments  were  car- 
ried out  to  determine  the  relative  importance  of  internal 
nutrient  reserve  translocation  and  exogenous  nutrient 
uptake  during  the  regeneration  process.  Approximately 
60%  of  the  dry  body  weight  of  M.  gracillima  is  organic 
material.  Intact  animals  held  for  three  weeks  in  natural 
seawater  did  not  change  significantly  in  weight,  caloric 
content,  or  relative  concentration  of  protein,  carbohy- 
drates, or  lipids.  Intact  animals  held  for  three  weeks  in 
artificial  seawater  devoid  of  nutrients  lost  weight  and  ca- 
loric content.  The  rate  of  loss  was  rapid  initially,  but 
slowed  after  about  eight  days.  Animals  regenerated  in 
natural  seawater  lost  weight  initially,  then  regained  the 
lost  weight.  Animals  regenerated  in  artificial  seawater  lost 
weight  constantly  and  at  a  higher  rate  than  either  the  ar- 
tificial seawater  control  or  natural  seawater  regenerated 
animals.  All  weight  losses  were  attributable  to  significant 
changes  in  the  protein  and  carbohydrate  fractions  of  the 
organic  body  component.  The  lipid  fraction  and  ash 
components  did  not  change  significantly  in  any  treatment. 
M.  gracillima  appears  to  be  adapted  to  regenerate  the  lost 
disk  rapidly,  even  under  conditions  of  food  deprivation. 

Introduction 

Autotomy  (self-mutilation  by  casting  off  body  parts), 
followed  by  regeneration  of  the  lost  parts,  is  widespread 

Received  26  February  1990;  accepted  26  November  1990. 
Contribution  #830  from  the  Belle  W.  Baruch  Institute  for  Marine 
Biology  and  Coastal  Research. 
*  To  whom  all  correspondence  and  reprint  requests  should  be  sent. 


among  the  echinoderms,  and  these  animals  are  known  to 
have  superb  wound-healing  and  regenerative  capacities 
(see  review  by  Emson  and  Wilkie,  1980;  Brown,  1982). 
The  few  published  studies  of  echinoderm  regeneration 
have  dealt  almost  exclusively  with  the  capacity  of  an  in- 
dividual species  to  regenerate,  descriptions  of  the  appear- 
ance of  new  structures,  or  measurements  of  regeneration 
rates  (Gibson  and  Burke,  1983).  Although  two  recent 
studies  have  estimated  the  environmental  energy  produc- 
tion represented  by  regenerating  brittlestar  arms  (Dui- 
neveld  and  Van  Noort,  1986;  O'Conner  el  al.,  1986),  the 
energetic  costs  to  the  regenerating  animal  of  autotomy, 
and  the  sources  of  nutrition  for  regeneration  in  echino- 
derms, have  not  been  evaluated. 

Members  of  at  least  five  families  of  ophiuroid  echino- 
derms (brittlestars)  autotomize  arms  or  the  aboral  disc 
(including  digestive  tract,  gonads,  and  disc  epithelium) 
when  disturbed.  They  can  regenerate  these  tissues  within 
a  few  weeks  in  the  laboratory  (Emson  and  Wilkie,  1980; 
pers.  obs.).  The  rate  of  tissue  replacement  must  be  related 
to  the  amount  of  stored  nutrients  available  and  the  rate 
at  which  the  animal  can  accumulate  and  allocate  addi- 
tional nutrients  for  tissue  regeneration.  The  ophiuroid  disc 
begins  to  regenerate  before  the  gut  has  been  replaced,  but 
the  sources  of  the  nutrients  that  support  that  process  are 
uncertain.  To  date,  no  specific  nutrient  storage  organ  other 
than  the  disc  has  been  found,  although  the  interstices  of 
the  arm  ossicles  may  be  repositories  for  nutrients  (Turner 
and  Murdoch,  1976).  The  ability  of  many  echinoderms 
to  translocate  nutrients  during  gametogenesis  (Lawrence, 
1987)  suggests  that  the  same  mechanism  could  be  used 
during  regeneration.  Echinoderms  may  also  take  up  dis- 


167 


168 


W.  E.  DOBSON  ET  .11. 


solved  organic  matter  (DOM)  from  the  surrounding  sea- 
water,  or  process  exogenous  paniculate  nutrients  by  ex- 
ternal digestion  (Lawrence,  1987;  Clements,  1988). 

Theoretically,  use  of  either  DOM  alone  or  external 
digestion  alone  would  result  in  a  net  gain  of  organic  matter 
by  regenerating  animals  with  no  concomitant  tissue  loss 
from  non-regenerating  portions  of  the  body.  In  contrast, 
internal  nutrient  translocation  would  result  in  the  decrease 
of  tissue  in  non-regenerating  portions  of  the  animal,  with 
no  net  gain  of  organic  matter  during  regeneration.  There 
should  even  be  a  net  loss  of  tissue  due  to  catabolism  of 
tissue  constituents  for  respiration  during  regeneration.  But 
translocation  of  stored  nutrients,  external  digestion,  and 
DOM  uptake  are  not  mutually  exclusive  and  may  operate 
sequentially  or  simultaneously.  Other  echinoderms  lack- 
ing special  storage  organs  resorb  body  parts  under  star- 
vation conditions  (Ebert,  1967;  Feral,  1985;  Lawrence, 
1987).  We  hypothesize  that  early  disc  regeneration  (prior 
to  reformation  of  the  functional  gut)  relies  heavily  on 
translocation  of  internal  nutrient  stores  that  are  mobilized 
from  the  non-regenerating  somatic  tissues.  The  purpose 
of  this  paper  is  to  estimate  the  relative  contribution  of 
internal  nutrient  translocation  to  disc  regeneration. 

Material  and  Methods 

Individuals  of  Microphiopholis  gracillima  were  col- 
lected from  intertidal  mud  flats  in  the  North  Inlet  Estuary 
just  north  of  Georgetown,  South  Carolina  (37°20rN, 
79°  10'W).  After  collection,  animals  were  taken  to  the  lab- 
oratory and  sorted  to  eliminate  all  but  individuals  with 
complete  (or  almost  completely  regenerated)  discs.  Ani- 
mals were  then  placed  in  autoclaved  all-glass  aquaria  in 
an  environmental  chamber  held  at  25  °C  with  a  12:12 
light:dark  cycle.  The  aquaria  contained  Millipore-filtered 
(0.45  /urn)  natural  seawater  (30%o).  All  aquaria  were  con- 
stantly aerated.  The  seawater  was  changed  daily  to  control 
bacterial  contamination  (Clements  et  ai,  1988).  Field- 
collected  animals  may  have  large  differences  in  their  nu- 
tritional states;  therefore  all  animals  were  allowed  to  ac- 
climate to  the  above  conditions  for  seven  days  before  the 
start  of  the  experiments  to  help  equalize  nutritional  dif- 
ferences. Animals  were  then  randomly  assigned  to  exper- 
imental groups. 

The  amount  of  nutrients  translocated  during  regener- 
ation was  estimated  in  the  following  treatments.  Intact 
and  autotomized  individuals  were  held  in  autoclaved  all- 
glass  aquaria  containing  either  Millipore-filtered  (0.45  urn) 
natural  seawater  without  sediment;  artificial  seawater 
alone  (Cavanaugh,  1956;  trace  minerals  formula  5);  or 
artificial  seawater  with  approximately  125  ^mol/1  glucose, 
125  /imol/1  palmitic  acid,  and  12.5  /umol/1  of  each  of  21 
amino  acids  (alanine,  arginine,  asparagine,  aspartic  acid, 
cysteine,  cystine,  glutamic  acid,  glutamine,  glycine,  his- 


tidine,  isoleucine,  leucine.  lysine.  methionine,  phenylala- 
nine,  proline,  serine,  threonine,  tryptophan,  tyrosine,  and 
valine)  added,  for  a  total  DOM  concentration  of  approx- 
imately 513  ^mol/1.  This  represents  about  a  five-fold  in- 
crease over  natural  DOM  levels.  (Clements,  1988;  Wil- 
liams, 1975).  All  aquaria  were  constantly  aerated  and  kept 
in  an  environmental  chamber  at  25°C  with  a  12:12  light: 
dark  lighting  regimen.  All  media  were  changed  daily.  At 
4-day  intervals,  10  animals  were  removed  from  each 
treatment  and  dissected  into  the  following  body  fractions: 
proximal,  medial,  and  distal  thirds  of  the  arms,  the  oral 
frame,  and  the  regenerated  (or  intact)  discs.  All  of  these 
fractions  were  dried  to  constant  weight  in  vacua  over  an- 
hydrous calcium  carbonate.  About  half  of  each  fraction 
was  ashed  at  400°C  for  6  h  and  its  ash-free  dry  weight 
determined.  The  remaining  three  parts  were  subjected  to 
biochemical  analyses  for  protein,  carbohydrate,  and  lipid, 
respectively.  Before  biochemical  analysis,  each  part  was 
split  into  three  replicates,  weighed,  and  ground  to  a  dry 
powder  in  a  hand-held  all-glass  homogenizer.  In  this  way, 
replicated  estimates  of  protein,  carbohydrate,  and  lipid 
were  obtained  for  each  body  fraction,  as  well  as  estimates 
of  ash-free  dry  weight  and  caloric  content.  Because  the 
tissue  samples  were  small,  colorimetric  techniques  were 
used  in  the  biochemical  analyses. 

Total  carbohydrate  levels  (as  reducing  sugars)  were  es- 
timated by  the  phenol-sulfuric  acid  method  of  Dubois  et 
ul.  (1956).  with  a  1:1  glucose:maltose  solution  as  the  stan- 
dard. Total  proteins  were  quantified  by  the  Bio-Rad 
(Richmond,  CA)  modification  of  the  Bradford  (1976) 
method,  with  a  1 : 1  mixture  of  bovine  serum  albumin  and 
purified  mollusk  protein  (Sigma)  as  the  standard.  Lipids 
were  extracted  from  the  fraction  homogenate  with  a  2:1 
(v:v)  chloroform:methanol  solution.  The  extract  was  pro- 
cessed according  to  the  sulphophosphovanillin  method  of 
Barnes  and  Blackstock  (1973),  with  purified  Microphio- 
pholis gracillima  lipid  as  the  standard.  The  purified  stan- 
dard was  prepared  according  to  the  procedures  outlined 
in  Barnes  and  Blackstock  (1973).  To  compensate  for  the 
possibility  of  significant  variation  in  total  weights  of  the 
specimens  used  in  this  study,  and  to  control  for  the  in- 
evitable loss  of  tissue  during  the  fractioning  of  the  samples 
for  the  different  biochemical  assays,  all  biochemical  mea- 
sures are  reported  as  units  per  gram  dry  weight  of  specimen 
rather  than  as  absolute  quantities  per  body  part. 

To  determine  the  relative  translocation  rate  of  proteins, 
carbohydrates,  and  lipids  during  regeneration,  300  ani- 
mals were  incubated  in  artificial  seawater  with  either  I4C- 
leucine  (specific  activity  348.0  mCi/mmol),  14C-glucose 
(3.5  mCi/mmol),  or  MC-palmitic  acid  (850.0  mCi/mmol) 
added  at  concentrations  of  0.03  ^Ci/ml,  0.04  ^Ci/ml,  and 
0.04  yuCi/ml,  respectively.  After  48  h,  the  animals  were 
removed  from  the  medium  and  rinsed  in  several  changes 
of  artificial  seawater.  One  half  of  the  animals  in  each  nu- 


NUTRIENT  TRANSLOCATION  DURING  OPHIURO1D  DISC  REGENERATION 


169 


trient  treatment  were  induced  to  autotomize  following 
the  procedure  of  Dobson  ( 1984,  1985).  Five  animals  from 
each  of  the  six  treatments  were  immediately  processed 
(see  below).  The  remaining  specimens  were  held  in  au- 
toclaved  all-glass  aquaria  containing  constantly  aerated 
artificial  seawater  in  an  environmental  chamber  at  25°C 
with  a  12:12  light:dark  cycle.  The  seawater  was  changed 
daily.  At  4-day  intervals,  for  20  days.  8  animals  were  re- 
moved from  each  treatment  and  dissected  to  separate  the 
distal  half  of  the  arms,  the  proximal  half  of  the  arms  (in- 
cluding the  oral  frame),  and  the  regenerated  (or  intact) 
disc  tissue.  Each  fraction  of  five  individuals  was  dried  to 
constant  weight  at  80°C  and  placed  in  a  separate  glass 
scintillation  vial  containing  1  ml  of  1:1  ProtoSol  (New 
England  Nuclear)  tissue  solublizenethanol.  Five  ml  of 
AquaSol  liquid  scintillation  cocktail  (New  England  Nu- 
clear) was  added  to  each  solublized  specimen  vial,  and 
the  samples  were  counted  with  a  Beckman  liquid  scintil- 
lation counter  with  internal  quench  correction.  The  other 
three  individuals  were  dried  to  constant  weight  at  80°C, 
ashed  at  400°C  for  6  h,  and  weighed  again.  We  normalized 
all  counts  by  computing  the  counts  per  minute  (CPM) 
per  gram  dry  weight  and  per  gram  ash-free  dry  weight  of 
the  tissue. 

All  data  were  analyzed  by  one-way  or  two-way  AN- 
OVA,  Tukey's  multiple  comparison  procedure  (Ostle  and 
Mensing,  1975;  Sokal  and  Rohlf,  1981),  or  least-squares 
linear  regression  using  the  General  Linear  Models  Pro- 
cedure of  the  Statistical  Analysis  System  (Carey,  North 
Carolina).  The  probability  of  making  any  type  I  error  at 
all  in  the  entire  series  of  tests  was  held  at  a  =  0.05  or  less 
[=  Experimentwise  error  rate  (Sokal  and  Rohlf,  1981, 
pg.  241)]. 

Results 

Biochemical  composition  of  the  intact  hri/t/es/ar 

Normal  values  for  organic  and  inorganic  constituents 
of  whole  and  individual  regions  of  Microphiopholis  gra- 
ci/lima  were  obtained  by  pooling  all  of  the  initial  (time 
=  0)  biochemical  measurements  from  each  experiment. 
The  results  are  summarized  in  Table  I.  About  60%  of  the 
total  dry  body  weight  is  organic  tissue  (as  ash-free  dry 
weight),  and  most  of  it  is  located  in  the  arms.  The  central 
disc  has  the  highest  organic  content  (74%)  relative  to  in- 
organic material,  but  this  represents  only  7%  of  the  total 
dry  body  weight  and  10%  of  the  total  organic  tissue  weight. 
The  proximal,  medial,  and  distal  arm  parts  and  the  oral 
frame  region  contain  50  to  60%  organic  material,  which 
accounts  for  90%.  of  the  total  organic  material.  The  arms 
have  a  higher  percentage  of  organic  material  at  their  bases 
than  at  their  tips. 

The  central  disc  and  oral  frame  have  higher  concen- 
trations (per  gram  dry  weight)  of  all  organic  components 


than  do  any  of  the  arm  regions.  The  disc  has  the  highest 
concentration  (per  gram  dry  weight)  of  protein  and  lipid, 
whereas  the  oral  frame  has  the  highest  concentration  of 
carbohydrates.  All  arm  fractions  are  similar  in  their  pro- 
tein, carbohydrate,  and  lipid  concentrations.  Interestingly, 
the  assayed  total  protein,  carbohydrate,  and  lipid  content 
of  the  body  accounts  for  only  30%  of  the  total  ash-free 
dry  weight  (=  organic  content)  of  the  brittlestar.  The  rel- 
ative underrepresentation  of  organic  material  is  constant 
between  body  fractions  with  the  exception  of  the  oral 
frame,  which  has  a  relatively  lower  underrepresentation. 
Most  of  the  total  missing  organic  material  is  located  in 
the  arm  parts.  Although  colorimetric  assays  commonly 
underestimate  the  actual  amount  of  material  present 
(Dubois  eta/.,  1956;  Barnes  and  Blackstock,  1973;  Davis, 
1988),  the  magnitude  of  the  underrepresentation  in  this 
case  is  unusual.  We  assume  that,  as  has  been  reported  for 
other  echinoderms  (Geise,  1966:  Feral,  1985),  the  majority 
of  the  missing  material  represents  insoluble  organic  ma- 
terial (such  as  connective  tissue),  organics  tied  up  in  the 
stromal  spaces  of  the  ossicles,  complexed  biochemicals 
(e.g..  glycoproteins  and  lipoproteins)  that  were  not  de- 
tected by  the  assays,  and  nucleic  acids. 

Change  in  biochemical  composition  of  tissues 
during  regeneration 

Bodv  weight  changes.  The  changes  in  total  dry  weight 
(DW).  total  organic  weight  (=  ash-free  dry  weight,  AFDW) 
and  total  inorganic  material  weight  (=ASH)  fractions  with 
time  in  individuals  in  the  natural  seawater  control  (NC), 
artificial  seawater  control  (AC),  natural  seawater  regen- 
erated (NR),  and  artificial  seawater  regenerated  (AR) 
treatments  are  shown  in  Figure  1.  Animals  in  artificial 
seawater  with  added  organics  did  not  survive  the  experi- 
ment and  thus  were  not  analyzed.  Animals  in  the  NC 
group  did  not  exhibit  any  significant  change  in  total  DW 
(P  =  0.3919),  ASH  weight  (P  =  0.9406),  or  AFDW  (P 
=  0.4805)  during  the  course  of  the  experiment  (Fig.  1  A). 
AC  animals  showed  a  rapid  initial  drop  in  both  total  DW 
(P  =  0.0466)  and  AFDW  (P  =  0.0002)  until  day  eight, 
after  which  both  weight  measures  remained  relatively 
constant.  Total  ASH  weight  did  not  change  significantly 
at  any  time  in  the  AC  group  (P  =  0.0828)  (Fig.  IB).  NR 
animals  did  not  lose  significant  amounts  of  total  DW  (P 
=  0.0546)  or  ASH  weight  (P  =  0.4458)  with  time,  but 
gradually  lost  AFDW  (P  =  0.0022)  until  about  day  12, 
after  which  AFDW  gradually  increased  through  day  20 
(Fig.  1C).  The  NR  and  AC  groups  lost  as  much  as  40% 
of  their  initial  AFDW  values  at  some  point  during  the  20- 
day  experiment.  AR  animals  displayed  a  rapid  initial  drop 
in  total  DW  (P  <  0.000 1 )  and  AFDW  (P  <  0.000 1 )  from 
day  0  to  day  4,  followed  by  a  slower  constant  decrease  in 
these  values.  The  maximum  loss  of  DW  during  the  ex- 


170 


W.  E.  DOBSON  ET  AL 


Table  I 


Normal  hiachcmical  iv>»i/"w"""'  "/  Microphiopholis  gracillima 


Constituent 

Whole 

Disc 

Body 

Part 

Distal  arms 

Oral  frame 

Proximal 
arms 

Medial  arms 

DRY  WEIGHT 

91.32  ±  10.87 

8.85  ±  1.44 

29.27  ±  3.57 

27.50  ±  4.76 

22.32  ±  4.71 

2.26  ±  0.26 

(%  total  body  part  DW) 

100 

100 

100 

100 

100 

100 

(%  whole  DW) 

100 

9.7 

32 

30.0 

24.5 

2.4 

ASH-FREE  DRY  WEIGHT 

52.47  ±    7.78 

6.52  ±  1.05 

15.96  ±  2.64 

16.32  ±  2.87 

12.35  ±  2.58 

1.35  ±0.22 

(%  total  body  part  DW) 

100 

74 

54.5 

59.3 

55.3 

59.7 

(%  whole  DW) 

58 

7.1 

17.5 

17.8 

13.5 

1.47 

(%  whole  AFDW) 

100 

12 

30.4 

31.1 

23.5 

2.5 

ASH  WEIGHT 

37.70  ±    4.81 

2.33  ±  0.42 

13.31  ±  1.30 

11.19  +  3.72 

9.97  ±  2.14 

0.91  ±0.14 

(%  total  body  part  DW) 

100 

26 

45.5 

40.6 

44.6 

40.3 

(%  whole  DW) 

42 

2.6 

14.6 

12.3 

10.9 

0.9 

(%  whole  Ash  weight) 

100 

6.2 

35.4 

29.7 

26.4 

2.4 

PROTEIN 

6.53  ±    0.35 

1.59  ±0.18 

2.04  ±  0.05 

1  .44  ±  0.03 

1.17  ±0.09 

0.21  ±0.01 

(%  total  body  part  DW) 

7.15 

18.02 

7.00 

5.27 

5.27 

9.60 

(%  whole  DW) 

7.15 

1.74 

2.23 

1.57 

1.28 

0.23 

(%  total  protein) 

100 

24.35 

31.24 

22.05 

17.91 

3.21 

CARBOHYDR.4TES 

2.88  ±    0.26 

0.74  ±  0.07 

0.9  1  ±  0.  1  3 

0.65  ±0.14 

0.33  ±  0.04 

0.11  ±  0.01 

(%  total  body  part  DW) 

3.15 

8.42 

3.08 

2.37 

1.48 

8.80 

(%  whole  DW) 

3.15 

0.81 

0.99 

0.71 

0.36 

0.12 

(%  total  carbohydrates) 

100 

25.69 

31.59 

22.56 

11.45 

3.81 

LIPIDS 

3.46  ±    0.33 

0.74  ±  0.05 

1.05  ±0.24 

0.94  ±  0.08 

0.60  ±0.1  7 

0.10  ±0.01 

(%  total  body  part  DW) 

3.79 

8.36 

3.58 

3.41 

2.68 

4.50 

(%  whole  DW) 

3.79 

0.81 

1.14 

1.02 

0.65 

0.11 

(%  total  lipids) 

100 

21.40 

30.60 

27.10 

17.60 

2.89 

UNACCOUNTED 

ORGANICS 

39.60  ±    3.89 

3.45  ±  0.09 

11.96  ±0.96 

13.29  ±  1.13 

10.25  ±0.76 

0.42  ±  0.02 

(%  total  body  part  DW) 

43.36 

38.90 

40.86 

48.30 

45.93 

18.58 

(%  whole  DW) 

43.36 

3.77 

13.09 

14.50 

11.22 

0.46 

(%  whole  AFDW) 

65.47 

6.57 

22.79 

25.32 

19.53 

0.80 

(%  total  UO) 

100 

8.71 

30.20 

33.56 

25.88 

1.06 

CALORIC  CONTENT  (calc.) 

0.93  ±    0.04 

2.40  ±  0.06 

0.83  +  0.08 

0.70  ±  0.03 

0.58  ±  0.08 

1.08  ±0.07 

(kCal/gdry  weight) 

All  values  are  averages  ±  one  standard  deviation.  All  units  are  milligrams  unless  otherwise  noted. 
DW  =  Dry  Weight,  AFDW  =  Ash-Free  Dry  Weight,  UO  =  Unaccounted  Organics. 


periment  occurred  in  this  group,  which  lost  as  much  as 
40%  of  the  initial  DW  and  50%  of  the  initial  AFDW.  Ash 
weight  did  not  change  significantly  in  the  AR  group  (P 
=  0.4893)  (Fig.  ID). 

When  the  total  DW  measurements  were  broken  down 
by  body  part,  the  following  trends  were  observed.  In  the 
NC  group,  no  significant  DW  change  occurred  in  any 
body  part  with  time  (P  >  0.05  in  all  fractions)  (Fig.  2A). 
In  the  AC  group,  the  DW  of  the  medial  (P  =  0.4247)  and 
proximal  (P  =  0.4928)  regions  of  the  arms  remained  rel- 
atively constant,  but  the  disc,  distal  arm  regions,  and  oral 
frame  lost  DW  until  about  day  eight,  after  which  their 
dry  weights  remained  constant  (P  =  0.0003,  P  =  0.0029, 
P  =  0.0025,  respectively)  (Fig.  2B).  Animals  in  the  NR 
group  exhibited  no  overall  change  in  DW  in  any  body 
part  (P  >  0.05)  after  first  appearance  of  the  disc  tissue, 
although  the  weight  of  the  oral  frame  on  day  16  was  sig- 


nificantly different  from  all  other  days.  Animals  in  the 
AR  group  lost  DW  throughout  the  experiment  in  all  non- 
regenerating  body  parts  (P  <  0.05).  This  loss  was  rapid 
until  approximately  day  eight,  after  which  the  decline 
proceeded  at  a  slower  rate. 

Ash-free  dry  weight  and  ASH  weight  measurements  by 
body  part  with  time  indicate  that  the  loss  in  DW  is  due 
to  loss  exclusively  from  the  organic  fraction  (Fig.  3).  There 
were  no  significant  changes  in  the  ASH  weights  of  any 
body  parts  in  any  experimental  treatment  over  the  20-day 
period  with  the  exception  of  first  appearance  of  the  discs 
(between  days  zero  and  four)  in  the  regenerating  groups 
(Fig.  4).  There  was  no  significant  change  in  AFDW  in 
anybody  part  with  time  (P  >  0.05)  in  the  NC  group  (Fig. 
3A).  In  the  AC  group,  all  body  parts  with  the  exception 
of  the  proximal  arm  fractions  lost  AFDW  until  approx- 
imately day  eight,  after  which  AFDW  remained  relatively 


NUTRIENT  TRANSLOCATION  DURING  OPHIUROID  DISC  REGENERATION 


171 


01 
OOP 
0.08 
0.07 
0.06 
005 
004 
0.03 
0.02 
0.01 
0 


(A)  Natural  Seawater  -  Control 


(B)  Artificial  Seawaler  ~  Control 


AFDW    •  DW  A  ASH 


0.1 

0.09 

0.08 
0.07 
0.06 
0.05  • 
0.04- 
0  03  • 
0.02- 
0,01  - 
0 


34  8  12  16 

TIME  (Days) 

(C)  Natural  Seawater  •  Regenerating 


20 


-I 


0 


8  12 

TIME  (Days) 


16 


ID)  Ajtitical  Seawater  -  Regenerating 


4  8  12 

TIME  (Days) 


Figure  1 .  Total  body  weight  changes  during  early  disc  regeneration.  (A)  Natural  Seawater  control  group. 
(B)  Artificial  seawater  control  group.  (C)  Natural  seawater  regenerating  group.  (D)  Artificial  seawater  regen- 
erating group.  Error  bars  represent  95%  confidence  intervals.  Error  bars  and  points  offset  slightly  for  graphical 
clarity. 


constant  in  all  body  fractions  (Fig.  3B).  The  most  rapid 
drop  in  AFDW  occurred  between  day  zero  and  day  four. 
Although  the  proximal  arm  fractions  did  lose  AFDW  over 
the  course  of  the  experiment,  the  loss  was  not  significant 
at  any  time  (P  =  0.1443).  Animals  in  the  NR  group  lost 
AFDW  from  all  non-regenerating  body  fractions  until  ap- 
proximately day  12,  after  which  AFDW  increased  (Fig. 
3C).  Because  of  the  high  variability  in  the  data,  the  changes 
in  AFDW  of  the  proximal  and  medial  arm  fractions  were 
not  statistically  significant  from  day  zero  at  any  other  time 
(P  =  0.0566  and  P  =  0.0853,  respectively).  The  AFDW 
of  all  non-regenerating  body  part  fractions  in  the  AR  group 
declined  continuously  until  day  16  of  the  experiment  (P 
<  0.05)  (Fig.  3D).  The  most  rapid  decrease  occurred  be- 


tween day  zero  and  day  four,  except  in  the  proximal  arm 
regions,  where  tissue  was  lost  at  a  constant  rate.  The  disc 
tissue  in  both  the  NR  and  AR  groups  did  not  increase  in 
AFDW  content  significantly  after  first  appearing. 

Protein  content  changes.  The  changes  in  total  body 
protein  concentration  over  time  are  shown  in  Figure  5A. 
The  natural  seawater  control  group  did  not  change  in 
total  protein  concentration  over  the  course  of  the  exper- 
iment (P  =  0.4717).  The  artificial  seawater  control  group 
exhibited  a  slight  decline  in  protein  concentration  with 
time  (P  =  0.0 1 2 1 ),  but  the  only  day  that  was  significantly 
different  from  the  others  in  this  group  was  day  eight.  The 
groups  regenerating  in  natural  seawater  and  in  artificial 
seawater  both  changed  slightly  in  total  protein  concen- 


172 


W.  E.  DOBSON  El  AL 


0.03 


(A)  Natural  Seawaler  -  Control 


D  DISTAL"  DISC   O  PROXIMAL 


0.03 


(B)  Artificial  Seawater  -  Control 


0.026  - 

_  O-022 
O  0.018 

i 

O  0.014 

E 


1 i 1 I 1       Q 


0.00 


0.03 


0.026 


0.018 

00 

O  0.014 


0.01 


0.006 


0.002 
0 


4  8  12          16          20 

TIME  (Days) 

(C)  Natural  Seawaler  -  Regenerating 


0006 


0,002 
0 


0.03 


•t- 


4  8  12          16          20 

TIME  (Days) 


(D)  Artifical  Seawater  -  Regenerating 


0  4  8  12          16          20 

TIME  (Days) 

Figure  2.  Changes  in  dry  weights  by  body  parts  during  early  disc  regeneration.  (A)  Natural  seawater 
control  group.  (B)  Artificial  seawater  control  group.  (C)  Natural  seawater  regenerating  group.  (D)  Artificial 
seawater  regenerating  group.  Error  bars  represent  95%  confidence  intervals.  Error  bars  and  points  offset 
slightly  for  graphical  clarity. 


tration  over  the  course  of  the  experiment  (P  <  0.001  in 
both).  The  protein  concentration  increased  at  the  same 
rate  in  both  the  NR  and  AR  groups  until  day  eight,  after 
which  the  NR  group  continued  to  gradually  increase  while 
the  AR  group  began  to  decline.  By  the  end  of  20  days, 
the  protein  concentration  in  the  AR  group  was  the  same 
as  its  initial  (day  zero)  protein  concentration. 

The  change  in  protein  concentration  over  time  by 
treatment  group  and  body  part  is  shown  in  Figure  6.  There 
was  no  change  in  protein  concentration  in  any  body  part 
in  the  NC  group  over  the  course  of  the  experiment  (P 
>  0.05)  (Fig.  6 A).  The  AC  group  lost  protein  in  significant 
amounts  from  the  disc  (P  =  0.0012),  distal  arm  fractions 
(P  <  0.0001 ),  and  oral  frame  (P  <  0.0001 ).  The  protein 


concentration  of  the  medial  and  proximal  arm  fractions 
did  not  change  (P  =  0.0675,  P  =  0.7822.  respectively) 
(Fig.  6B).  There  was  no  change  in  the  protein  concentra- 
tion of  any  arm  fractions  in  the  NR  group  (P  >  0.05),  but 
the  oral  frame  lost  significant  amounts  of  protein  relative 
to  its  dry  weight  (P  =  0.0003),  while  the  disc  rapidly  in- 
creased in  protein  concentration  (P  <  0.0001)  (Fig.  6C). 
The  AR  treatment  group  lost  protein  from  all  non-regen- 
erating body  parts  (P  <  0.05)  (Fig.  6D).  The  protein  con- 
centration of  the  disc  in  the  AR  group  increased  rapidly 
until  day  12  (P  <  0.0001),  then  fell  off  rapidly  through 
day  20  (P  <  0.0001).  The  rate  of  increase  to  day  12  was 
the  same  as  in  the  NR  treatment  (P  >  0.05).  The  rate  of 
protein  loss  from  the  oral  frame  was  slower  in  the  AR 


NUTRIENT  TRANSLOCATION  DURING  OPHIUROID  DISC  REGENERATION 


173 


0.017 


(A)  Natural  Seawater  -  Control 


0.007 


0.005 
0.003 

0001 
0 


H-4-H 


D  DISTAL"  DISC  O  PROXIMAL 


OR  ALA  MEDIAL 


u.ui  /  - 

0015- 

§0013- 

I 

s 

•"----, 

^ 

£2  0011  - 

/ 

X 

"**.^ 

o 

"*-  —  „ 

5  ooofl- 

# 

t 

g  0.007  - 

1 

"-, 

LU 

r\ 

E  0.005- 

\ 

X 

i 

. 

\ 

<  0.003 

0.001 
n 

Ji 

r 

\ 
\ 

,--^ 

d 

4  8  12          16         20 

TIME  (Days) 

(C)  Natural  Seawaier  -  Regenerating 


4  8  12          16          20 

TIME  (Days) 

(D)  Arttfical  Seawaier  -  Regenerating 


u.ui  /  - 
0.015  • 

•N. 

V'.Ul  /  " 

0015  • 

PX. 

. 

v^ 

V 

/ 

'   \ 

\ 

0.013  • 

\ 

\ 

/-' 

1  0.013- 

1 

\ 

• 

!      \ 

,v 

\ 

^ 

' 

1 

-,    \ 

•s. 

0011  • 

\ 

,->^ 

f' 

2  o.oi  i 

1  '\  l> 

N, 

• 

1 

o 

| 

X. 

0.009- 

g  0.009- 

'--. 

X^^  

£ 

- 

i 

-  ^ 

0.007 
0.005 

i 

'"1 

r 

g  0007  - 

LU 

£  0  005  - 

•    -    1  

. 

X 

0.003 

<  0.003  - 

0.001 

0.001  - 

*  i 

'               *              « 

i 

1  1 

i  —  i 

n  - 

«-  1 

i-  1 

0  4  8  12          16         20  0  4  8  12          16         20 

TIME  (Days)  TIME  (Days) 

Figure  3.  Changes  in  ash-free  dry  weights  by  body  parts  during  early  disc  regeneration.  (A)  Natural 
seawater  control  group.  (B)  Artificial  seawater  control  group.  (C)  Natural  seawater  regenerating  group.  (D) 
Artificial  seawater  regenerating  group.  Error  bars  represent  95%  confidence  intervals.  Error  bars  and  points 
offset  slightly  for  graphical  clarity. 


and  NR  groups  than  in  the  AC  group  (P  <  0.0001),  but 
loss  occurred  throughout  the  experiment,  whereas  the  AC 
group  stopped  losing  protein  from  the  oral  frame  at  about 
day  8.  The  AR  and  NR  treatment  groups  lost  protein 
from  the  oral  frame  at  the  same  rate  throughout  the  ex- 
periment (P  =  0.0931).  The  AR  treatment  lost  protein 
from  the  distal  arms  at  a  higher  rate  than  did  the  AC 
treatment  group  (P  <  0.0001 ). 

Carbohydrate  content  changes.  The  results  of  the  total 
body  carbohydrate  assays  are  graphed  by  day  in  Figure 
5B.  The  NC  and  NR  groups  did  not  exhibit  any  significant 
change  in  total  carbohydrate  concentration  with  time  (P 
=  0.0877,  P  =  0.4784).  The  AC  and  AR  groups  did  exhibit 
changes  in  total  carbohydrate  concentration  (P  =  0.0063, 


P  <  0.0001 )  over  the  course  of  the  experiment.  There  was 
no  difference  in  the  rate  of  loss  between  the  AC  and  AR 
groups  (P  =  0.5675). 

The  changes  in  carbohydrate  concentration  of  the  var- 
ious body  parts  with  time  in  the  different  treatments  is 
graphed  in  Figure  7.  The  NC,  AC,  and  NR  groups  lost 
significant  amounts  of  carbohydrates  only  from  the  oral 
frame  (P  =  0.01 73,  P  =  0.0002.  P  =  0.0075,  respectively). 
Although  there  were  fluctuations  in  the  carbohydrate 
concentration  of  the  other  non-regenerating  body  parts 
in  each  of  these  groups,  they  did  not  represent  significant 
changes  in  concentration  with  time  (Fig.  7 A,  B,  C).  The 
AR  group  lost  significant  amounts  of  carbohydrates  from 
the  distal  and  medial  arm  parts  and  the  oral  frame  (P 


174 


W.  E.  DOBSON  ET  AL. 
(A)  Natural  Seawaler  -  Conlrol  (Bl  Artificial  Seawaler  -  Conlrol 


0.016- 

0.014- 

0012 

I    °°' 

00 

£  0.008 

^ 

1 

uuio- 
0.014- 

0.012 
1     001 

i 

X  0.008 

pj 

=^ 

"**i 

,^y 

.  —  • 

hi 

-( 

x  °  °°6 

D  DISTAL*  DISC 

O  PROXIMAL 

1 

0.004 

•  OR  ALA  MEDIAL                  Q(m 

1              ; 



0.002 

o- 

C 

L~±±: 

.--T-H 

>  •  1 

0002 

0 

0                   ( 

\-^\'^ 

4            8           12          16          2 

4            8            12           16          2( 

TIME  (Days)                                                                    TIME  (Days) 

0.016 


(C)  Natural  Seawater  -  Regenerating 


(D)  Artiftcal  Seawaler- Regenerating 


8  12 

TIME  (Days) 


TIME  (Days) 


Figure  4.  Changes  in  ash  weights  by  body  parts  during  early  disc  regeneration.  (A)  Natural  seawater 
control  group.  (B)  Artificial  seawater  control  group.  (C)  Natural  seawater  regenerating  group.  (D)  Artificial 
seawater  regenerating  group.  Error  bars  represent  95"i  confidence  intervals.  Error  bars  and  points  offset 
slightly  tor  graphical  clarity. 


<  0.05).  The  proximal  arm  parts  did  not  exhibit  a  signif- 
icant change,  although  there  appeared  to  be  a  gradual 
decline  in  carbohydrate  concentration  (P  =  0.0623,  Fig. 
7D).  Both  the  NR  and  AR  groups  exhibited  a  rapid  in- 
crease in  the  carbohydrate  content  of  the  regenerating  disc, 
with  rate  of  increase  being  the  same  in  both  groups  through 
day  12.  After  day  12,  the  disc  continued  to  increase  in 
carbohydrate  concentration  in  the  NR  group,  while  the 
carbohydrate  concentration  in  the  disc  tissue  of  the  AR 
group  began  to  decline.  The  rate  of  decline  in  oral  frame 
carbohydrate  concentration  was  identical  across  all  treat- 
ments (P  >  0.05)  until  day  20,  when  the  NC  treatment 
was  different  from  the  AC,  NR,  and  AR  treatments,  which 
were  still  the  same  (P  <  0.05). 


Lipid  content  changes.  The  changes  in  total  body  lipid 
concentration  over  time  are  shown  in  Figure  5C.  There 
were  no  significant  changes  in  the  total  lipid  concentration 
within  any  of  the  treatments  with  time  (P  >  0.05).  Between 
treatments,  the  NC  and  AC  treatments  were  identical  on 
all  days.  In  addition,  the  NR  and  AR  treatments  were 
identical  through  day  12.  The  NR  treatment  was  different 
from  the  AR  treatment  and  the  same  as  the  NC  and  AC 
treatments  at  day  16.  All  treatments  had  the  same  lipid 
concentrations  by  day  20. 

The  changes  in  lipid  concentration  by  day  and  body 
part  are  illustrated  in  Figure  8.  There  was  no  significant 
change  in  lipid  concentration  in  any  non-regenerating 
body  part  in  the  NC,  AC,  and  NR  groups  (P  >  0.05)  (Fig. 


NUTRIENT  TRANSLOCATION  DURING  OPHIUROID  DISC  REGENERATION 


175 


(A)  TOTAL  BODY  PROTEIN 
(All  Treatments) 


» 

a 


o 
£ 


I 

t 

Q 


go- 

go- 

|             1-  -+--£"! 

70- 
60- 

50 

^  ~  s 

40 

f—  K 

30 
20 
10 
n  ^ 

y  T          *      —  4 

D    NAT-CONTQ   ART-CONT 
•    NAT-REG     •    ART-REG 

(B)  TOTAL  BODY  CARBOHYDRATE 
(All  Treatments) 


50 


40- 


30 


20 


1(1 


f 

O 

I 


4  8  12          16          20 

TIME  (Days) 

(C)  TOTAL  BODY  LIPID 

(All  Treatments) 


4=44 


t- 

Q 


8  12 

TIME  (Days) 


16 


20 


4  8  12 

TIME  (Days) 

(D)  TOTAL  BODY  CALORIC  CONTENT 
(All  Treatments  -  Averages) 


Figure  5.  Total  body  biochemical  concentration  changes  during  early  disc  regeneration.  (A)  Total  body 
protein  by  treatment  group.  (B)  Total  body  carbohydrate  by  treatment  group.  (C)  Total  body  lipid  by 
treatment  group.  (D)  Total  body  caloric  content  (calculated)  by  treatment  group.  Error  bars  represent  95% 
confidence  intervals.  Error  bars  and  points  offset  slightly  for  graphical  clanty. 


8A,  B,  C).  Although  the  AC  and  NR  groups  showed  a 
constant  decline  in  lipid  concentration  in  all  body  parts 
except  the  regenerating  disc  of  the  NR  treatment,  the 
overall  changes  were  not  statistically  significant.  The  AR 
group  showed  a  significant  decrease  in  lipid  concentration 
in  the  medial  arm  fraction  (P  =  0.0235)  as  well  as  the 
same  non-significant  concentration  decline  in  all  other 
non-regenerating  body  parts  shown  by  the  AC  and  NR 
groups.  The  NR  and  AR  groups  exhibited  rapid  increases 
in  lipid  concentration  in  the  disc  tissue  fragment,  which 
were  the  same  through  day  16  (P>  0.05).  The  NR  group 
had  a  higher  lipid  concentration  in  the  disc  fraction  by 
day  20  (P  =  0.2430). 


Caloric  content  changes.  Caloric  values  presented 
here  were  calculated  from  the  biochemical  data  using 
caloric-conversion  values  (protein,  5.65  kcal/g;  carbo- 
hydrate, 4.10  kcal/g;  lipid,  9.45  kcal/g;)  (Brody,  1964; 
Ekert  and  Randall,  1978).  Although  the  current  trend 
in  physiological  research  is  to  use  the  SI  unit  of  energy 
(joules),  we  determined  energy  content  as  calories  and 
present  the  data  here  in  calories  for  ease  of  comparison 
with  previous  literature.  However,  one  calorie  equals 
4.184  joules  (Crisp,  1984),  so  direct  conversion  between 
units  is  relatively  simple.  Although  there  is  potential 
for  error  in  using  calculated  values  instead  of  real  values 
for  caloric  content  (Giese,  1966;  Cummins  and  Wuy- 


176 


W.  E.  DOBSON  £T  .•)! 


(A)  Natural  Seawater  -  Control 


(B)  Artificial  Seawaler  •  Control 


zuu  • 

^MU  - 

180- 

1  SO- 

160 

li,  II- 

1 

140 

-^ 

-*--*--+--< 

I 

140- 

^ 

120 

$ 

120- 

b 

£• 

c 

Q 

bo 

100 

bo 

100 

c. 

. 

"s 

V 

5 

80 

*                         A                    —A-                      4 

V 

0 

80 

. 

*  *  --     < 

jn^S,. 

fi 

bo 

E 

60 

^ 

-4--K      N, 

B 

60 

40 

I—  -1 

u                                                                    _1 

40 

^--v            ^  -^.k-  i 
^—l^^^fci^l 

*        *"§  8-  3  ( 

20 

D  DISTAL*  DISC   O  PROXIMAL 

20 

•  ORAL  A  MEDIAL 

0 

-f-- 

u  T  1  1  1  1  1 

0            4            8            12           16          20 

0            4            8           12           16          2 

TIME  (Days) 

TIME  (Days) 

(C)  Natural  Seawaler  -  Regenerating 


(D)  Artificial  Seawater  -  Regenerating 


180- 

| 

160 

1    •"  i"-4 

f        <• 

140- 

!          \ 

120- 

\ 

100 

/                         ' 

i 

80 

*  —  0- 

^__^^^4--  —  i  |  

60  • 

^^t1        4>       4 

40- 

!              '"•*  <r~:~-V-~~' 

20- 

n  - 

, 

4  8  12 

TIME  (Days) 


4  8  12 

TIME  (Days) 


20 


Figure  6.  Protein  content  changes  by  body  part  during  early  disc  regeneration.  (A)  Natural  seawater 
control  group.  (B)  Artificial  seawater  control  group.  (C)  Natural  seawater  regenerating  group.  (D)  Artificial 
seawater  regenerating  group.  Error  bars  represent  95%  confidence  intervals.  Error  bars  and  points  offset 
slightly  for  graphical  clarity. 


check,  1971;  Feral,  1985).  the  calculated  caloric  values 
are  probably  closer  to  the  "true"  values  than  the  actual 
calorimetry  data  due  to  procedural  errors  in  obtaining 
the  micro-bomb  calorimetry  data  and  the  resulting  wide 
variations  in  the  actual  caloric  values.  The  total  cal- 
culated caloric  content  of  the  body  in  the  different 
treatments  is  illustrated  in  Figure  5D.  With  the  excep- 
tion of  day  four  in  the  AC  group  (which  was  only  dif- 
ferent from  day  20),  there  were  no  statistically  signifi- 
cant differences  in  caloric  content  with  time  in  either 
of  the  NCand  AC  groups  (P>  0.05).  The  caloric  content 
of  the  NR  and  AR  groups  declined  constantly  (P 
<  0.0001),  with  the  AR  group  losing  caloric  content 
faster  than  the  NR  group  (P  <  0.0001). 


The  caloric  content  changes  with  time  by  body  part  are 
diagrammed  in  Figure  9.  The  natural  seawater  control 
group  lost  calories  only  in  the  disc  fraction  (P  =  0.0310) 
(Fig.  9A).  All  other  body  parts  maintained  their  caloric 
levels  (P  >  0.05).  The  artificial  seawater  control  group 
lost  calories  only  from  the  disc  and  oral  frame  (P  =  0.0045, 
P  =  0.0050).  not  the  arm  fractions  (P  >  0.05)  (Fig.  9B). 
The  NR  treatment  group  lost  calories  from  the  oral  frame 
and  distal  arm  fractions  (P  =  0.001 7,  P  =  0.0064)  but  not 
the  medial  and  proximal  arm  fractions  (P  >  0.05)  (Fig. 
9C).  The  AR  group  lost  calories  from  every  non-regen- 
erating body  part  (P  <  0.0001)  (Fig.  9D).  The  NR  and 
AR  groups  both  increased  the  caloric  content  of  their  disc 
tissue  until  day  16.  By  day  20.  the  AR  group  had  begun 


NUTRIENT  TRANSLOCATION  DURING  OPHIUROID  DISC  REGENERATION 


177 


(A)  Natural  Seawaler  -  Control 


(B)  Artificial  Seawaler  -  Control 


50- 

P 

\       ll 

40 

fi 

-~4^ 

u.  —  - 

,.-' 

40 

fTH=N 

a 

-N.,,*-  "*" 

X 

«x 

S 

/ 

1 

*       30 

S        30 

£• 

•—  

(  ( 

e 

Q 
be 

|      20 

h—  ~i 

FH-+- 

<- 

.-I 

Q 

so 

|       20 

-,r^^^ 

-t—  -i__. 

V) 

c/l 

bo 

,          *                ..-•?•• 

bo 

.                         ', 

Q 

s 

11  *P                 "  (    4 

10 

D  DISTAL"  DISC   O  PROXIMA 

10 

•  ORAL  A  MEDIAL 

n 

II 

TIME  (Days) 
(C)  Natural  Seawaler  •  Regenerating 


0  4  8  12  16          20 

TIME  (Days) 

(D)  Artificial  Seawater  -  Regenerating 


4  8  12  14          20 

TIME  (Days) 


10 


4  8 

TIME  (Days) 


Figure  7.  Carbohydrate  content  changes  by  body  part  during  early  disc  regeneration.  (A)  Natural  seawater 
control  group.  (B)  Artificial  seawater  control  group.  (C)  Natural  seawater  regenerating  group.  (D)  Artificial 
seawater  regenerating  group.  Error  bars  represent  95%  confidence  intervals.  Error  bars  and  points  oftset 
slightly  for  graphical  clarity. 


to  lose  calories  from  the  disc  tissue,  whereas  the  NR  group 
continued  to  add  calories  to  the  disc  tissue.  The  rate  of 
increase  in  caloric  content  was  the  same  in  the  NR  and 
AR  groups  through  day  12. 

Rate  of  nutrient  translocation 

All  brittlestars  took  up  statistically  significant  amounts 
of  l4C-leucine  and  '4C-glucose  during  the  pulse  portion 
of  the  experiment  (Fig.  10,  1 1).  Counts  of  the  individual 
body  parts  indicated  that  all  body  parts  absorbed  label  in 
approximately  the  same  quantities  per  gram  of  dry  body 
weight  (P  =  0.5740).  However,  the  animals  only  accu- 
mulated significant  amounts  of  '4C-palmitic  acid  in  the 


disc  region  of  the  body.  This  result  was  somewhat  unex- 
pected, because  other  echinoderms  are  known  to  take  up 
lipids,  especially  exogenous  palmitic  acid,  from  their  en- 
vironment across  their  dermal  surfaces  (Beijnink  and 
Voogt,  1984). 

During  the  post-absorption  portion  of  the  experiment, 
l4C-leucine  and  l4C-glucose  label  counts  decreased  rapidly 
and  in  approximately  linear  fashion  in  all  the  experimental 
treatments  (Fig.  10,  11).  14C-palmitic  acid  concentration 
changes  were  not  followed  because  the  animals  failed  to 
take  up  the  material  in  non-regenerating  body  parts. 
Counts  of  the  individual  body  parts  indicated  that  14C- 
leucine  and  l4C-glucose  labels  were  lost  in  approximately 
the  same  proportions  from  all  non-regenerating  fractions 


178 


W.  E.  DOBSON  ET    11. 


(A)  Natural  Seawaler  -  Control 


(B)  Artificial  Seawaler  -  Control 


IS 

t 

Q 


I 

I 
t 

O 


90- 
80 

H-+-+-H 

70- 

60 

50- 
40- 

tfc3=tid| 

30 

20- 

1  M  }^:l 

10- 
n  - 

D  DISTAL  •   DISC     O  PROXIMAI 
•  ORAL  A  MEDIAL 

0  4  8  12          16         20 

TIME  (Days) 


(C)  Natural  Seawaler  -  Regenerating 


4  8  12  16          20 

TIME  (Days) 

(D)  Artificial  Seawater  -  Regenerating 


4  8  12 

TIME  (Days) 


16 


20 


4  8  12 

TIME  (Days) 

Figure  8.  Lipid  content  changes  by  body  part  during  early  disc  regeneration.  (A)  Natural  seawater  control 
group.  (B)  Artificial  seawater  control  group.  (C)  Natural  seawater  regenerating  group.  (D)  Artificial  seawater 
regenerating  group.  Error  bars  represent  95%  confidence  intervals.  Error  bars  and  points  offset  slightly  for 
graphical  clarity. 


of  the  body,  including  the  disc  of  intact  specimens.  How- 
ever, little  of  the  label  lost  from  the  non-regenerating  tis- 
sues of  regenerating  animals  was  incorporated  into  the 
regenerating  disc  tissue.  Although  counts  of  the  regener- 
ating disc  tissue  showed  that  some  radiolabel  was  incor- 
porated into  the  disc  tissue,  the  levels  were  not  significantly 
different  from  background  counts  throughout  the  course 
of  the  experiment  (P  >  0.05). 

Discussion 

The  experimental  treatments  used  to  study  the 
amount  of  nutrients  translocated  during  disc  regener- 
ation can  be  described  in  terms  of  nutrient  availability. 


The  NC  group  represented  control  animals  that  were 
given  access  to  dissolved  organic  material  (DOM),  but 
not  paniculate  food,  to  determine  the  effect  of  mainte- 
nance metabolism  on  the  body's  biochemical  compo- 
sition when  both  stored  nutrient  catabolism  and  DOM 
uptake  were  available  as  energy  sources.  The  AC  group 
represented  control  animals  that  had  to  rely  on  stored 
nutrients  alone  to  supply  energy  for  maintenance.  The 
NR  group  were  animals  that  had  to  supply  energy  for 
both  maintenance  metabolism  and  regeneration,  as  well 
as  building  materials  for  regeneration.  These  animals 
had  access  to  both  stored  nutrients  and  DOM  uptake 
sources  of  nutrients.  The  AR  group  represented  animals 
that  had  to  both  maintain  metabolism  and  regenerate 


NUTRIENT  TRANSLOCATION  DURING  OPHIUROID  DISC  REGENERATION 


179 


(A)  Natural  Seawater  -  Control 


(B)  Anificial  Seawarer  -  Conirol 


U 
X 


2.2- 

1.8 

K  •*--«-  +  -  f  -< 

14 

f  ^ 

•r                                             -4- 

0.6 

p  J  7.1.7.          ."/I      ^ 

CU- 
ii- 

D  DISTAL"  DISC   O  PROXIMAL 
•  ORAL  A  MEDIAL 

2.6 


2.2- 


1  4> 


0.6 


r--.t 


1 


4  8  12  16          20 

TIME  (Days) 

<C)  Natural  Seawaler  -  Regenerating 


0  4  8  12  16          20 

TIME  (Days) 

(D)  Anificial  Seawater  -  Regenerating 


z.o  • 

Z-D  ' 

2.2- 

^ 

2.2- 

18- 

I/-T 

1  8 

§ 
'« 

/|" 

1 

/K 

fr       14- 

. 

E-      1.4 

i 

Q 

Q 

* 

bO 

.K 

60 

/ 

5       i 
u 

u: 

0.6- 

^T  ^-^--^-H 

5    1 

u 
w 

0.6- 

^  It-~f--^-H 

'~              N                            ^^  —  ^                   <t                  I 

1    -    T        -4-          4          4          4 

/                "  *  ^-     <f 

>~--(-4~.  "t~"i---^--  ] 

1          •"*"•--+  :j 

02- 
0  • 

• 

0.2- 

n 

• 

0            4            8            12           16          20                     0 

4           8           12          16         2( 

TIME  (Days)                                                                     TIME  (Days! 

Figure  9.  Tissue  caloric  content  changes  by  body  part  during  early  disc  regeneration.  (A)  Natural  seawater 
control  group.  (B)  Anificial  seawater  control  group.  (C)  Natural  seawatei  regenerating  group.  (D)  Artificial 
seawater  regenerating  group.  Error  bars  represent  95%  confidence  intervals.  Error  bars  and  points  offset 
slightly  for  graphical  clarity. 


in  the  absence  of  any  external  nutrient  source  (i.e..  only 
stored  nutrients  were  available). 

Under  natural  seawater  control  conditions,  animals 
survived  for  at  least  four  weeks  (one  week  of  acclimation 
plus  three  weeks  of  experimentation)  with  no  significant 
change  in  the  overall  biochemical  composition  of  the 
body.  The  only  localized  changes  in  body  constituents 
occurred  in  the  oral  frame  region,  which  lost  small 
amounts  of  carbohydrates  during  the  experiment.  Al- 
though the  animals  probably  lost  stored  nutrients  from 
all  body  parts  under  these  conditions,  the  losses  were  below 
the  limits  of  detection.  The  total  energy  content  of  the 
animal  did  not  change.  The  only  localized  caloric  content 
change  occurred  in  the  disc  region  and  could  not  be  at- 


tributed to  changes  in  any  measured  biochemical  com- 
ponent. This  indicates  that,  although  animals  deprived  of 
particulate  food  may  be  stressed,  they  probably  are  not 
starving  (i.e.,  they  are  obtaining  nutrients  by  direct  uptake 
from  the  environment).  This  result  is  consistent  with  pre- 
vious studies  showing  that  echinoderms,  including  brit- 
tlestars,  can  obtain  up  to  58%  of  their  energetic  require- 
ments from  DOM  (Ferguson,  1982a,  b;  Feral,  1985;  Law- 
rence, 1987;  Clements,  1988). 

When  deprived  of  all  exogenous  food  (artificial  seawater 
treatments),  control  animals  initially  lost  stored  material 
at  a  rapid  rate.  The  material  was  lost  from  the  disc,  oral 
frame,  and  distal  arm  regions  of  the  body,  and  was  at- 
tributable to  losses  of  protein  and  carbohydrates,  but  not 


180 


W.  E.  DOBSON  ET  AL 


(A)  Natural  Seawater  Experiment 


(B)  Artificial  Seawater  Experiment 


•  AR-D1ST 
*•  AR-PROX 

•  AC-DIST 
O   AC-PROX 
D>  AC-DISC 
O   AR-DISC 


NR-DIST 
NR-PROX 
•    NC-DIST 

n   NC-PROX 

NC-DISC 
O    MR-DISC 


TIME  (Days)  TIME  (Days) 

Figure  10.  14C-Leucine  tracer  content  of  tissues  during  early  disc  regeneration.  (A)  Natural  seawater 
experiment.  (B)  Artificial  seawater  experiment.  AC  =  Artificial  seawater  control,  AR  =  Artificial  seawater 
regenerating,  NC  =  Natural  seawater  control,  NR  =  Natural  seawater  regenerating,  DIST  =  Distal  arm, 
PROX  =  proximal  arm  and  oral  frame.  Error  bars  represent  95%  confidence  intervals.  Error  bars  and  points 
offset  slightly  for  graphical  clarity. 


lipids.  The  loss  of  total  caloric  content  in  the  various  body 
parts  followed  the  same  pattern,  with  no  significant  loss 
in  any  other  body  parts.  After  four  days,  these  animals 
appeared  to  acclimate  to  the  lack  of  food  such  that  the 
rate  of  overall  materials  loss  was  reduced;  (i.e.,  they  ap- 
parently adjusted  to  food  deprivation  by  reducing  their 
consumption  of  stored  material).  The  temporal  pattern 
of  material  loss  may  also  represent  a  rapid  initial  use  of 
stored  resources  followed  by  a  breakdown  of  essential  body 
tissues  to  maintain  metabolism.  As  tissue  mass  decreased, 
the  metabolic  load  due  to  those  tissues  decreased,  and  the 
rate  of  tissue  loss  declined.  Because  mass-specific  meta- 
bolic rates  were  not  obtained  during  this  experiment,  these 
observations  could  not  be  empirically  verified. 

There  are  two  possible  explanations  for  the  spatial  pat- 
tern of  material  loss  in  the  artificial  seawater  control  group. 
The  first  is  that  the  disc,  oral  frame,  and  arm  tips  are 
preferentially  resorbed  when  the  animal  is  forced  to  ca- 
tabolize  tissue  for  maintenance.  Turner  and  Murdoch 


(1976)  described  such  a  pattern  of  arm  tissue  loss  during 
regeneration  of  the  disc  in  Ophiophragmus  filograneus. 
This  mechanism  would  leave  the  majority  of  the  arm  tis- 
sue undisturbed  so  that  normal  feeding  activity  would  not 
be  impaired  when  feeding  conditions  improved.  The  sec- 
ond possibility  is  that  the  absolute  rate  of  loss  is  the  same 
from  all  body  parts,  but  there  is  less  material  in  the  disc, 
oral  frame,  and  arm  tips  to  begin  with,  so  the  material 
available  within  them  is  exhausted  sooner  than  that  in 
other  body  parts.  The  latter  possibility  is  the  more  likely, 
because  the  medial  and  proximal  arms  have  the  highest 
total  amounts  of  all  biochemical  constituents  (Table  I). 

Animals  regenerating  in  natural  seawater  showed  an 
initial  decrease  in  organic  mass  followed  by  a  gradual  in- 
crease. This  indicates  that  the  use  of  material  during  early 
regeneration  exceeded  the  rate  at  which  DOM  uptake 
from  the  medium  could  compensate  for  it,  and  thus  must 
have  been  at  least  partially  independent  of  external  nu- 
trient availability.  Loss  of  organic  material  occurred  in 


NUTRIENT  TRANSLOCATION   DURING  OPHIUROID  DISC  REGENERATION 


181 


(A)  Natural  Seawater  Experiment 


(B)  Artificial  Seawater  Experiment 


Q  i 

II 

cu 

U 


.1 
I 


u 


60 


50- 


40- 


30- 


20- 


10- 


40- 
30- 

20- 
10- 


0 


1  NR-DIST 

>  NR-PROX 
•  NC-DIST 
n  NC-PROX 

>  NC-DISC 
O  MR-DISC 


0 


16 


20 


TIME  (Days) 


£    ii 


0. 

U 


Q 
-S" 

cu 
U 


TIME  (Days) 


Figure  11.  '4C-Glucose  tracer  content  of  tissues  during  early  disc  regeneration.  (A)  Natural  seawater 
experiment.  (B)  Artificial  seawater  experiment.  AC  =  Artificial  seawater  control,  AR  =  Artificial  seawater 
regenerating,  NC  =  Natural  seawater  control.  NR  =  Natural  seawater  regenerating,  DIST  =  Distal  arm, 
PROX  =  proximal  arm  and  oral  frame.  Error  bars  represent  95%  confidence  intervals.  Error  bars  and  points 
offset  slightly  for  graphical  clarity. 


all  non-regenerating  body  parts,  but  the  overall  trend  was 
similar  to  that  exhibited  by  the  artificial  seawater  control 
group  in  that  most  of  the  loss  was  from  the  oral  frame 
and  arm  tips.  The  subsequent  increase  in  organic  material 
appeared  to  be  localized  in  the  arms.  The  relative  protein 
content  of  the  body  increased  constantly  during  regen- 
eration, indicating  either  a  net  gain  of  protein  during  re- 
generation, or  a  loss  of  minerals  as  tissue  breakdown  oc- 
curred. This  gain  could  be  due  to  a  net  uptake  of  proteins 
(or  amino  acids)  from  the  medium,  or  a  combination  of 
uptake  and  overall  loss  of  other  body  biochemical  con- 
stituents during  regeneration.  Although  the  carbohydrate 
and  lipid  content  of  the  body  did  not  change  significantly 
over  the  same  period,  the  latter  explanation  is  more  likely, 
because  the  animals  lost  total  caloric  content  constantly 
during  regeneration.  The  increase  in  organic  material  in 
the  non-regenerating  portions  of  the  body  after  day  12  is 
problematical,  because  no  corresponding  increase  in  bio- 
chemical constituents  in  those  parts  could  be  demon- 


strated. This  increase  might  be  explained  as  the  summa- 
tion of  non-significant  increases  in  each  biochemical  con- 
stituent to  make  a  significant  increase  in  total  organics. 

Animals  regenerating  in  the  absence  of  exogenous  nu- 
trients constantly  lost  organic  material  from  non-regen- 
erating body  parts.  The  rate  of  loss  was  relatively  rapid 
through  day  8,  and  slower  from  day  12  through  day  20. 
This  change  was  related  to  the  constant  decrease  in  pro- 
tein, carbohydrate,  and  lipid  content  of  the  non-regen- 
erating tissues.  Although  lipid  content  loss  was  statistically 
significant  only  in  the  medical  portions  of  the  arms,  all 
non-regenerating  body  parts  showed  a  trend  toward  lipid 
loss.  The  regenerating  disc  tissue  increased  in  protein, 
carbohydrate,  and  lipid  content  through  day  12,  after 
which  protein  and  carbohydrate  content  dropped  dra- 
matically, while  lipid  content  remained  the  same  or 
slightly  increased  (the  continued  proportional  lipid  in- 
crease was  probably  due  to  the  loss  of  protein  and  car- 
bohydrates). The  caloric  content  of  these  animals  dropped 


182 


W.  E.  DOBSON  ET  AL 


constantly,  and  consistently  faster  than  that  in  the  exper- 
imental group  regenerating  in  natural  seawater.  All  non- 
regenerating  parts  of  the  body  lost  calories  throughout  the 
experiment.  The  caloric  content  of  the  disc  increased 
through  day  16,  then  dropped  dramatically. 

The  data  on  the  consumption  of  hiochemicals  and  disc 
tissue  production  in  the  regeneration  experimental  groups, 
especially  the  artificial  seawater  regeneration  group,  seems 
to  indicate  that  the  process  of  regeneration  runs  at  a  set 
rate,  and  may  be  independent  of  the  nutritional  state  of 
the  animal  (at  least  for  the  first  two  weeks  of  disc  regen- 
eration). A  similar  phenomenon  has  recently  been  re- 
ported in  crinoids  under  field  conditions  (Meyer,  1988). 
These  observations  imply  that  early  replacement  of  initial 
disc  tissues  and  structures  has  priority  over  the  mainte- 
nance of  body  mass.  Since  these  observations  coincide 
temporally  with  appearance  of  the  functional  gut  (Dobson 
and  Stancyk,  in  prep),  one  can  conclude  that  the  animal 
tries  to  replace  the  gut  so  it  can  feed  again  regardless  of 
its  initial  nutritional  state.  Only  when  resources  drop  be- 
low some  critical  level  (i.e.,  the  actual  onset  of  starvation) 
do  they  stop  regenerating  the  disc.  This  experiment  should 
be  repeated  with  animals  that  have  been  held  without 
food  sources  for  varying  lengths  of  time  to  determine 
whether  regeneration  is  even  initiated  after  the  critical 
point  in  the  food  withdrawal  period  has  passed. 

Regeneration  appears  to  require  a  set  amount  of  nu- 
trients, which  are  transported  from  the  deep  tissues  of  all 
the  non-regenerating  body  parts.  If  food  is  present  (as 
DOM  in  this  case)  the  loss  of  material  due  to  translocation 
may  be  offset  by  uptake.  Further,  after  the  gut  lining  is 
reformed  and  becomes  functional,  ingestion  of  particu- 
lates,  including  small  bacteria,  may  ameliorate  the  loss  of 
stored  nutrients. 

A  previous  attempt  to  verify  and  quantify  nutrient 
translocation  into  the  disc  from  somatic  body  parts  during 
disc  regeneration  in  Al.  gracillima  was  unsuccessful 
(Clements,  1988).  That  study  relied  on  the  assumption 
that  loss  of  organic  material  from  the  arms  would  result 
in  a  decrease  in  total  arm  size.  This  assumption  was  based 
on  the  results  of  Turner  and  Murdoch  (1976)  and  the 
observation  that  echinoid  test  diameter  decreases  during 
starvation  (Ebert,  1967).  However,  a  loss  of  arm  tissue 
without  a  reduction  in  overall  arm  size  has  been  dem- 
onstrated in  starving  asteroids  (Lawrence  et  al..  1986). 
Thus,  the  internal  soft  tissues  of  asteroid  arms  are  scav- 
enged while  leaving  the  calcified  structures  in  place.  In- 
deed, the  arms  of  asteroids  have  been  implicated  as  general 
nutrient  storage  organs  (Beijnink  and  Voogt,  1984;  Law- 
rence, 1987).  If  the  non-regenerating  body  parts  of  M. 
gracillima  are  fulfilling  a  similar  role,  then  translocation 
of  organic  material  from  the  non-regenerating  body  parts 
should  occur  without  an  overall  decrease  in  body  part  size 
or  inorganic  (  =  ASH)  weight.  The  calcification  of  tissues 


in  marine  invertebrates  is  also  a  relatively  expensive  pro- 
cess compared  to  the  production  of  soft  tissues,  due  to 
the  energetics  of  mineralization  and  the  cost  of  producing 
the  skeletal  matrix  (Simkiss,  1976;  Palmer,  1983;  Law- 
rence, 1987).  Consequently,  we  would  expect  the  calcified 
structures  of  M.  gracillima  to  be  conserved  even  as  its 
soft  tissues  are  degraded  to  supply  catabolic  and  regen- 
erative nutrients.  Because  the  entire  external  surface  of 
M.  gracillima  is  covered  with  plate  ossicles  and  spines, 
the  shape  and  size  of  body  parts  would  not  change  much 
as  the  soft  tissues  are  degraded  inside  the  structures.  The 
absence  of  change  in  the  ash  weight  of  all  the  body  parts 
of  all  animals  in  the  current  study  supports  this  hypothesis. 

Abnormal  regeneration  and  death  of  specimens  in  the 
nutrient-enriched  experimental  groups  is  perplexing,  but 
has  been  verified  by  repeated  experimentation  (Clements. 
1988;  K.  Fielman,  pers.  comm.).  Because  preliminary  ex- 
periments indicated  that  these  conditions  promoted  bac- 
terial growth  (Clements,  1988),  we  took  care  to  inhibit 
such  growth  by  completely  changing  the  medium  each 
day.  Several  researchers  have  proposed  that  echinoderm 
regeneration  requires  the  presence  of  functional  nerve  fi- 
bers that  produce  recognition  and  regulatory  molecules 
(Bisgrove  et  al..  1988;  P.  Mladenov,  pers.  comm.).  Ab- 
normally high  ambient  concentrations  of  nutrients  (es- 
pecially amino  acids,  which  can  act  as  neurotransmitters) 
may  have  directly  affected  the  regeneration  process  by 
interfering  with  the  actions  of  these  recognition  molecules. 

Uptake  of  14C-leucine  and  l4C-glucose  indicated  that 
dissolved  organic  material  is  taken  up  in  statistically  sig- 
nificant amounts  in  all  treatments.  The  results  agree 
closely  with  those  of  Clements  (1988)  for  net  uptake  of 
the  amino  acids  leucine  and  glycine  by  M.  gracillima. 
However,  her  study  showed  significant  retention  of  the 
labeled  compound  over  time.  The  current  results  indicate 
that  the  initially  retained  labeled  molecules  are  rapidly 
turned  over  or  leaked  back  into  the  medium,  with  little 
permanent  incorporation  of  the  labeled  molecules  into 
the  tissues  and  no  translocation  of  the  labeled  material  to 
the  active  regeneration  site.  The  labeled  compounds  may 
have  been  transported  in  quantities  below  the  detected 
threshold  of  the  assay  method.  We  do  not  know  whether 
the  loss  of  label  from  non-regenerating  tissues  is  due  to 
leakage  or  respiration,  because  the  experiment  was  not 
designed  to  test  for  respired  I4CO2  or  for  an  increase  in 
the  label  content  of  the  medium  with  time.  We  would 
understand  this  process  better  if  the  '4C-leucine,  '4C-glu- 
cose,  and  I4CO:  evolved  in  the  medium  during  the  post- 
absorption  portion  of  the  experiment  had  been  assayed 
to  determine  what  fraction  of  the  material  taken  up  by 
the  animals  was  catabolized  or  leaked  out. 

The  absence  of  detectable  translocation  of  radiolabeled 
material  into  regenerating  tissue  indicates  that,  if  the  la- 
beled material  is  not  simply  leaking  out  of  the  body  [which 


NUTRIENT  TRANSLOCATION  DURING  OPHIUROID  DISC  REGENERATION 


183 


is  not  expected  to  be  the  case  based  on  the  results  obtained 
by  Clements  (1988)],  then  the  material  may  have  been 
absorbed  only  into  the  surface  tissue  layers  of  the  body, 
and  not  subsequently  transported  into  the  deeper  tissues. 
Several  investigators  have  proposed  such  DOM  absorption 
as  a  mechanism  by  which  echinoderms,  which  have  poor 
circulatory  systems,  maintain  their  external  tissues  (Fer- 
guson, 1982b;  Bamford,  1982).  In  these  models,  DOM 
feeds  the  external  tissues,  but  is  not  transported  into  the 
deep  tissues,  whereas  material  ingested  and  digested  is  not 
transported  to  the  surface  layers  but  supplies  nutrients 
only  to  the  internal  tissues.  Because  the  current  results, 
and  the  results  of  previous  work  on  regeneration  (Dobson 
and  Stancyk,  in  prep),  indicate  that  nutrients  are  trans- 
located from  the  deep  tissues  of  the  non-regenerating  body 
parts — probably  by  coelomocytes  of  the  water-vascular 
system — the  lack  of  label  in  the  regenerating  disc  may  be 
ascribed  to  its  inability  to  migrate  into  the  deep  tissues 
and  thus  to  be  available  for  regeneration. 

Disc  autotomy  is  probably  a  predator  avoidance  mech- 
anism (Turner  et  a/.,  1981).  Because  the  disc  (or  at  least 
the  gut)  is  needed  for  feeding,  some  mechanism  should 
be  available  to  replace  it  after  escape-response  disc  au- 
totomy, irrespective  of  the  nutritional  state  of  the  animal. 
Such  an  effect  has  been  demonstrated  in  this  and  a  pre- 
vious set  of  experiments  (Dobson,  Stancyk.  and  Clements, 
in  prep).  In  addition,  because  M.  gracillima  is  a  seasonal 
spawner  (pers.  obs.),  selection  for  rapid  replacement  of 
the  disc  structures  to  facilitate  replacement  of  gonads  and 
gametes  would  be  expected.  Because  these  animals  lose 
up  to  one-fourth  of  their  available  body  organic  mass  dur- 
ing early  regeneration,  a  massive  amount  of  body  reserves 
must  enter  the  process.  However,  a  significant  amount  of 
the  reserves  must  be  used  for  maintenance  metabolism. 
This  study  shows  that,  although  these  animals  do  have 
some  energy  storage  resources  (because  the  starving  and 
regenerating  animals  still  produce  disc  tissue),  there  is  still 
no  specific  nutrient  storage  organ  or  tissue.  Without  ad- 
ditional exogenous  nutrient  input,  these  stores  are  depleted 
within  about  two  weeks,  a  sufficient  time  for  replacement 
of  the  gut  and  initiation  of  feeding,  even  when  paniculate 
and  dissolved  exogenous  organic  material  is  absent. 

Acknowledgments 

This  work  would  not  have  been  possible  without  the 
facilities  of  the  Biology  Department  of  U.S.C.  and  the 
Belle  W.  Baruch  Institute.  This  work  was  supported  in 
part  by  a  Grant  from  the  Slocum-Lunz  Foundation  of 
South  Carolina  to  W.  E.  Dobson,  and  in  part  from  a  Na- 
tional Institute  of  Health  Biomedical  Research  Support 
Grant  (grant  #BSR-85 14326)  to  S.  E.  Stancyk,  W.  E. 
Dobson,  R.  M.  Showman,  and  L.  A.  Clements.  Field  as- 
sistance was  provided  by  K.  Zimmerman  and  K.  Fielman. 
Special  thanks  to  L.  F.  Dobson  for  gestalt  support. 


Literature  Cited 

Bamford,  D.  1982.  Epithelial  absorption.  Pp.  317-330  in  Echinoderm 
\ntrinoH.  M.  Jangoux  and  J.  M.  Lawrence,  eds.  A.  A.  Balkema. 
Rotterdam. 

Barnes,  H.,  and  J.  Blackstock.  1973.  Estimation  of  lipids  in  marine 
animals  and  tissues:  detailed  investigation  of  the  sulphophospho- 
vamllin  method  for  "total"  lipids.  J.  E\p.  Mar.  Biol.  Ecol.  12:  103- 
118. 

Beijnink,  F.  B.,  and  P.  A.  Voogt.  1984.  Nutrient  translocation  in  the 
sea  star:  whole-body  and  microautoradiography  after  ingestion  of 
radiolabeled  leucine  and  palmitic  acid.  Biol.  Bull.  167:  669-682. 

Bisgrove,  B.  W.,  P.  V.  Mladenov,  S.  K.  Asotra,  and  R.  D.  Burke. 
1988.  Wound  healing  and  regeneration  following  arm-tip  ampu- 
tation in  the  sea  star  Lefiiaxieruix  hcxactis.  P.  788  in  Echinoderm 
Biology.  Proceedings  ol  the  Sixth  International  Echinoderm  Confer- 
ence. R.  D.  Burke.  P.  V  Mladenov,  P.  Lambert,  and  R.  L.  Parsley, 
eds.  Victoria.  23-28  August. 

Bradford,  M.  M.  1976.  A  rapid  and  sensitive  method  for  the  quanti- 
fication of  protein  utilizing  the  principle  of  protein  dye-binding.  Anal 
Biochem  72:  248-254. 

Brody,  S.  1964.  Bioenergetics  and  Growth.  Hafner.  New  York.  1023 
pp. 

Brown,  B.  K.  1982.  Gut  replacement  during  disc  regeneration  of  the 
autotomized  disc  of  Ophiopliruginux  filograneus  (Echmodermata: 
Ophiuroidea).  Unpublished  master's  thesis,  Florida  Institute  of 
Technology.  Melbourne.  90  pp. 

Cavanaugh.G.  M.  1965.  Formulae  and  Methods  I',  of  the  Marine  Bio- 
logical Laboratory  Chemical  Room  Mar.  Biol.  Lab.,  Woods  Hole. 
Mass.  87  pp. 

Clements,  L.  A.  J.  1988.  Uptake  and  utilization  of  dissolved  free  amino 
acids  by  the  brittlestar  Microphiopholis  gracillima  (Say,  1852)  (Echi- 
nodermata:  Ophiuroidea).  Unpublished  Ph.D.  dissertation.  University 
of  South  Carolina.  1 16  pp. 

Clements,  L.  A.  J.,  Fielman,  K.  T.,  and  S.  E.  Stancyk. 
1988.  Regeneration  by  an  amphiund  bnttlestar  exposed  to  different 
concentrations  of  dissolved  organic  material.  J  Exp.  Mar  Biol.  Ecol. 
122:  47-61. 

Crisp,  D.  J.  1984.  Energy  flow  measurements.  Pp.  284-372  in  Methods 
for  the  Stiuly  of  Marine  Benthos.  N.  A.  Holme  and  A.  D.  Mclntyre. 
eds.  Blackwell  Scientific  Publications,  Oxford. 

Cummins.  K.  \V.,  and  Wuycheck,  J.  C.  1971.  Caloric  equivalents  for 
investigations  in  ecological  energetics.  Int.  Ass.  Theor.  App.  Limnol. 
Vol  18.  162  pp. 

Davis,  E.  M.  1988.  Protein  assays:  a  review  of  common  techniques. 
Am.  Biotech  Lab  5:  28-37. 

Dobson,  \V.  E.  1984.  Nervous  mediation  of  disc  autotomy  in  Ophio- 
phragmus  filograneus  (Echinodermata:  Ophiuroidea).  M.S.  Thesis. 
Florida  Institute  of  Technology,  Melbourne.  68  pp. 

Dobson,  W.  E.  1985.  A  pharmacological  study  of  neural  mediation  of 
disc  autotomy  in  Ophiophragmus  filograneus  (Lyman)  (Echino- 
dermata: Ophiuroidea).  /  Exp.  Mar  Biol.  Ecol.  94:  223-232. 

Dubois,  M.,  K.  A.  Gilles,  J.  K.  Hamilton,  P.  A.  Rebers,  and  F.  Smith. 
1956.  Colonmetnc  method  for  determination  of  sugars  and  related 
substances.  Anal  Chcm.  28:  350-356. 

Duineveld,  G.  C.  A.,  and  G.  J.  Van  Noort.  1986.  Observations  on  the 
population  dynamics  of  Amphnira  ftliformis  (Ophiuroidea:  Echi- 
nodermata) in  the  southern  North  Sea  and  its  exploitation  by  the 
Dab.  Limanda  limanda.  Neth.  J  Sea  Res.  20:  85-94. 
Ebert,  T.  A.  1967.  Negative  growth  and  longevity  in  the  purple  sea 
urchin  Slrongylocenlrolus  piirpuratux  (Stimpson).  Science  157:  557- 
558. 

Ekert,  R..  and  D.  Randall.  1978.  Animal  Physiology.  Freeman,  San 
Francisco.  558  pp. 


184 


W.  E.  DOBSON  ET  .41. 


Emson,  R.  H.,  and  I.  C.  Wilkie.  1980.  Fission  and  autotomy  in  echi- 
noderms.  Oceiuwgr.  Mar.  Bid.  Aim  Review  18:  155-250. 

Feral,  J.-P.  1985.  Effect  of  short-term  starvation  on  the  biochemical 
composition  of  the  apodous  holothurian  Lenlosynapla  gallicnnei 
(Echinodermata):  possible  role  of  dissolved  organic  material  as  an 
energy  source.  Mar.  BH//.  86:  297-306. 

Ferguson,  J.  C.  1982a.  A  comparative  study  of  the  net  metabolic  ben- 
efits derived  from  the  uptake  and  release  of  free  amino  acids  by  marine 
invertebrates.  Biol.  Bull.  162:  1-17. 

Ferguson,  J.  C.  1982b.  Support  of  metabolism  of  superficial  structures 
through  direct  uptake  of  dissolved  primary  amines  in  echinoderms. 
Pp.  345-351  in  Echinoderm  Biology:  Proceedings  of  the  International 
Echinoderm  Conference,  J.  M.  Lawrence,  ed.  Tampa  Bay,  14-17 
September. 

Gibson,  A.  W.,  and  R.  D.  Burke.  1983.  Gut  regeneration  by  mor- 
phallaxis  in  the  sea  cucumber  Leptosynapla  clarki  (Heding.  1928). 
Can.J.  Zoo/.  61:2720-2732. 

Giese,  A.  C.  1966.  On  the  biochemical  constitution  of  some  echino- 
derms. Pp.  757-796  in  Physiology  oj  Echinodermata,  R.  A.  Booloo- 
tian,  ed.  John  Wiley,  New  York. 

Lawrence,  J.  1987.  A  Functional  Biology  ol  Echinoderms.  Johns  Hop- 
kins University  Press,  Baltimore.  340  pp. 

Lawrence,  J.,  T.  S.  Klinger,  J.  B.  McClintock,  S.  A.  Watts,  C.  P.  Chen, 
A.  March,  and  L.  Smith.  1986.  Allocation  of  nutrient  resources  to 
body  components  by  regenerating  Lindia  clalhrala  (Say)  (Echino- 
dermata: Asteroidea).  J  E.\p.  Mar.  Biol.  Ecol.  102:  47-53. 

Myer,  D.  L.  1988.  Crinoids  as  renewable  resources:  rapid  regeneration 
of  the  visceral  mass  in  a  tropical  reef-dwelling  crinoid  from  Australia. 


Pp.  519-522  in  Echinoderm  Biology:  Proceedings  of  the  Sixth  Inter- 
national Eclunoderm  Conference,  R.  D.  Burke.  P.  V.  Mladenov,  P. 
Lambert,  and  R.  L.  Parsley,  eds.  Victoria,  23-28  August.  818  pp. 

O'Conner,  B.,  T.  Bowmer,  D.  McGrath,  and  R.  Raine.  1986.  Energy 
flow  through  an  Amphmra  /i/i/ormis  (Ophiuroidea:  Echinodermata) 
population  in  Galway  Bay,  west  coast  of  Ireland:  a  preliminary  in- 
vestigation. Ophelia  26:  351-357. 

Ostle,  B.  and  R.  \V.  Mensing.  1975.  Statistics  in  Research,  3rd  ed. 
Iowa  State  University  Press.  596  pp. 

Palmer,  A.  R.  1983.  Relative  cost  of  producing  skeletal  organic  matrix 
versus  calcification:  evidence  from  marine  gastropods.  Mar  Biol.  75: 
287-292. 

Simkiss,  K.  1976.  Cellular  aspects  of  calcification.  Pp  1-32  in  The 
Mechanisms  ol  Mineralization  in  the  Invertebrates  and  Plants,  N. 
Watabe  and  K.  M.  Wilbur,  eds.  Univ.  of  South  Carolina  Press. 

Sokal,  R.  R.,  and  F.  J.  Rohlf.  1981.  Biometry  Freeman.  New  York. 
859  pp. 

Turner,  R.  I,.,  and  J.D.  Murdoch.  1976.  Potential  of  arms  as  a  nutrient 
source  for  disc  regeneration  in  bnttlestars.  Am.  Zoo/.  16:  288  (Ab- 
stract). 

Turner,  R.  L.,  D.  VV.  Heatwole,  and  S.  E.  Stancyk.  1981.  Ophiuroid 
discs  in  stingray  stomachs:  evasive  autotomy  or  partial  consumption 
of  prey?  Pp.  331-335  in  Echinoderm  Biology:  Proceedings  of  the 
International  Echinoderm  Conference,  J.  M.  Lawrence,  ed.  Tampa 
Bay,  14-17  September. 

Williams,  P.  J.  LeB.  1975.  Biological  and  chemical  aspects  of  dissolved 
organic  material  in  seawater.  Pp.  301-363  in  Chemical  Oceanography, 
Vol.  2,  Rilev,  J.  P.  and  G.  Skirrow,  eds.  Academic  Press,  London. 


Reference:  Biol.  Bull  180:  185-195.  (February, 


Calcium-Proton  Exchange  During  Algal  Calcification 


TED  A.  McCONNAUGHEY1*   AND  RICHARD  H.  FALK2 

1  Marine  Biological  Laboratory,  Woods  Hole,  Massachusetts  02543,  and 2 Botany  Department, 
University  of  California,  Davis,  California  95616 


Abstract.  Extracellular  calcification  by  the  giant  celled 
alga  Chara  coral  Una  may  involve  active  Ca2^  extrusion 
from  the  cell  in  exchange  for  protons.  The  following  ev- 
idence is  presented:  CaCO,  incrustations  accrete  largely 
along  the  inside,  facing  the  cell,  as  revealed  by  X-ray  mi- 
croanalysis  using  Sr:+  and  Mn2+  as  tracers  for  new  min- 
eralization. Inward  proton  currents  are  inhibited  by  the 
Ca:+  transport  antagonists  Gdu  and  La34 .  Low  Ca2+  con- 
centrations inhibit  pH  banding  and  photosynthesis,  and 
solutions  of  low  Ca:+  activity  support  more  photosynthesis 
in  the  presence  of  additional  buffered  calcium.  The  ratio 
of  calcification  to  photosynthesis  in  moderately  alkaline 
solutions  containing  sufficient  calcium  remains  stable  at 
about  1.0  independent  of  solution  Ca24  concentration. 
Ion  specific  microelectrodes  placed  close  to  the  calcified 
surface  sometimes  detect  increases  in  Ca:+  activity  coin- 
cident with  decreases  in  proton  activity.  As  the  pCa  of 
solution  increases,  the  maximum  pH  observed  at  the  al- 
kaline surface  increases,  as  does  the  maximum  solution 
pH  which  supports  electrochemical  currents  by  the  cell. 
Combinations  of  extracellular  pH  and  pCa  approach  the 
calculated  thermodynamic  limits  for  ATP  driven  2H+/ 
Ca2+  exchange  against  the  cytosol. 

Introduction 

Ca2+  ATPase  appears  to  be  associated  with  calcification 
in  various  animals  and  plants  (e.g.,  Klaveness,  1976; 
Okazaki,  1 97  7;  Okazaki  eM/.,  1984;Kingsley  and  Watabe, 
1 985).  This  report  explores  the  possibility  that  extracellular 


Received  3  April  1990;  accepted  6  November  1990. 

*Address  communications  to:  Dr.  Ted  McConnaughey.  U.S.  Geolog- 
ical Survey,  Box  25046  MS  413,  Lakewood,  CO  80225. 

Abbreviations:  CAPS  =  3-(cyclohexylamino)propanesulfbnate;  CHES 

=    2-(N-cyclohexylamino)ethanesulfonate;    MOPS    =    2-(N-morpho- 

linolpropanesulfonate;  PIPES  =  l,4-piperazinediethane-sulfonate;TAPS 

tris(hydroxymethyl)methylaminopropane     sult'onate;     TRIS 

=  tris(hydroxymethyl)aminomethane. 


calcification  in  characean  algae  involves  active  calcium- 
proton  exchange. 

Characeans  calcify  as  a  by-product  of  bicarbonate  as- 
similation from  alkaline  waters  (Spear  et  al.,  1969;  Raven 
etal..  1986;  Okazaki  and  Tokita,  1988).  The  plants  extract 
proton  equivalents  from  the  medium  along  parts  of  their 
giant  cells,  forming  alkaline  patches  or  bands  that  may 
become  heavily  calcified.  The  proton  equivalents  are  ex- 
truded elsewhere,  forming  acidic  patches  or  bands.  There, 
HCOr  is  apparently  protonated  to  form  CO:,  which  the 
plant  absorbs  (Walker  el  al.,  1980;  Smith  and  Walker, 
1980;  Price  and  Badger,  198?).  Pericellular  carbonic  an- 
hydrase  and  complicated  invaginations  of  the  plasma 
membrane  within  the  acid  zones  may  facilitate  CO2  gen- 
eration and  absorbtion  (Price  el  al.,  1985). 

Characeans  can  be  more  than  half  CaCO3  by  dry  weight, 
and  as  will  be  shown  here,  calcification  is  often  stoichio- 
metric  to  photosynthesis.  Nevertheless,  calcification 
physiology  has  been  largely  neglected,  and  calcification  is 
generally  assumed  to  be  independent  of  active  Ca2+  trans- 
port (e.g.,  Raven  et  al..  1986).  Various  evidence  neverthe- 
less suggests  that  active  Ca2+  transport  might  be  involved. 
First,  Ca2+  ATPases  apparently  catalyze  Ca2+  extrusion 
from  cells  in  exchange  for  protons  (Niggli  et  al..  1982; 
Villalobo  and  Roufogalis,  1986;  Rasi-Caldogno  et  al., 
1987;  Dixon  and  Haynes,  1989).  Ca2+  ATPase  could 
therefore  catalyze  proton  uptake  at  the  site  of  calcification 
in  Chara.  Second,  characeans  are  functionally  analogous 
to  coccolithophorid  algae,  which  also  calcify  in  an  ap- 
proximate ratio  of  1 : 1  to  photosynthesis,  but  do  so  intra- 
cellularly  (e.g..  Sikes  et  al..  1980).  Ca2+  and  carbon  pre- 
sumably traverse  the  cytoplasm  to  reach  the  vesicular  site 
of  calcification,  and  Ca2+  ATPase  seems  to  be  involved 
(Klaveness,  1976;  Okazaki  et  al.,  1984).  Third,  molecular 
CO2  apparently  provides  most  of  the  precipitating  carbon 
during  calcification  by  various  plants  and  animals 
(McConnaughey,  I989a,  b,  c),  including  Chara  (Mc- 


185 


186 


T.  A.  McCONNAUGHEY  AND  R.  H.  FALK 


Connaughey,  in  prep).  Since  HCO3  is  more  abundant 
in  alkaline  solutions,  its  unimportance  in  calcification 
suggests  that  the  calcifying  region  can  be  fairly  isolated 
from  solution.  The  calcifying  cell  must  therefore  supply 
calcium,  and  remove  the  protons  generated  by  the  reaction 
Ca2+  +  CO2  +  H2O  =  CaCO3  +  2H+.  And  finally,  Ca2+ 
is  well  known  to  affect  characean  photosynthesis  and 
membrane  properties  associated  with  pH  banding  (e.g., 
Lucas,  1976;  Wiesenseel  and  Ruppert,  1977;  Luhring  and 
Tazawa,  1985;  Bisson,  1984;  Tazawa  el  a/..  1987). 

A  Ca:+  ATPase  model  and  a  more  conventional  proton 
channel  model  for  characean  calcification  are  illustrated 
in  Figure  1 .  Both  models  are  elaborated  to  fit  the  available 
data.  Ca2+  influx  into  the  cell,  in  the  Ca:+  ATPase  model, 
occurs  within  the  alkaline  band  (Fig.  Ib)  to  produce  the 
observed  electrogenic  character  of  pH  banding  (Walker 
and  Smith,  1977).  Figure  Ic,  e  shows  the  use  of  molecular 
COi  from  the  plant  as  the  major  carbon  source  for  cal- 
cification (McConnaughey,  in  prep.)  and  the  accretion  of 
extracellular  calcium  deposits  from  the  inside  (demon- 
strated here).  Figures  Id  and  Ifdepict  non-calcifying  con- 
ditions, such  as  when  Ca2+  or  carbon  levels  are  too  low 
to  sustain  much  CaCO^  precipitation.  The  non-calcifying 
condition  can  be  experimentally  useful,  because  the  H+ 


fluxes  measured  extracellularly  then  reflect  cellular  H  + 
transport  most  closely. 

The  energy  (E)  required  for  proton  uptake  under  both 
models  is  given  by: 

E  =  FV(aZCa  -  bZH)  +  RT  In  (Ca,/Cat,)a/(H,/H0)b     ( 1 ) 

The  terms  on  the  right  represent  work  done  against  the 
membrane  electrical  potential  V,  and  against  the  mem- 
brane chemical  gradients.  F  is  the  Faraday  constant,  "a" 
and  "b"  are  the  numbers  of  Ca2i  and  H+  ions  transported 
per  cycle,  Z  is  ionic  charge.  R  is  the  gas  constant,  and  T 
is  Kelvin  temperature.  Cytoplasmic  and  external  Ca2+  and 
H+  activities  are  subscripted  "i"  and  "o,"  respectively. 

For  the  proton  channel  model,  protons  are  drawn  into 
the  cell  by  the  membrane  electrical  potential.  The  ther- 
modynamic  limit  for  passive  (E  =  0)  proton  uptake  occurs 
when  the  membrane  electrical  and  chemical  gradient 
energies  balance,  yielding  a  proton  Nernst  equation: 

0  =  FV  +  2.3  RT(pH0  -  pH,)  (2) 

For  an  illuminated  cell  in  alkaline  solution,  the  membrane 
potential  might  be  around  —200  mV  and  cytoplasmic 
pH,  might  be  about  7.5-8.0  (Smith  and  Raven,  1979; 
Spanswick  and  Miller,  1977;  Mimura  and  Kirino,  1984; 


Ca" 


Alkaline 
Band 


2H 


CaCOj 


H  Channel 

CALCIFYING  NON-CALCIFYING 

•  Ca" 


H* Channel 


Ca  ATPase 


2  OH 


Figure  1.  Models  of  extracellular  calcification  and  its  coupling  to  bicarbonate  utilization  in  Chora.  Left: 
schematic  of  a  cell,  showing  alkaline  band  at  top  (with  trapezoidal  CaCO3  incrustations),  and  acid  band 
below  (with  plasmalemmasomes,  participating  in  bicarbonate  use).  "P"  represents  photosynthesis,  (a)  Proton 
channel  model.  HCO3~  diffuses  to  the  alkaline  surface  and  donates  a  proton,  becoming  converted  to  CO3", 
which  precipitates  with  Ca2+.  (b)  Ca2+  ATPase  model.  ATP  driven  2H+/Ca2+  exchange  alkalimzes  the  external 
medium  and  locally  increases  its  Ca2+  concentration.  CO2  diffuses  from  the  cell  and  reacts  with  water  to 
yield  the  protons  needed  for  exchange  with  Ca;+,  and  the  CO5"  which  precipitates  as  CaCOj.  A  1:1  ration 
of  calcification  to  photosynthesis  is  shown  for  both  models.  Right:  elaborations  on  the  H*  channel  and  Ca2+ 
ATPase  models  for  the  alkaline  band,  incorporating  inward  accretion  ofCaCO,  incrustations,  using  CO2  as 
the  carbon  source.  Proton  channel  (e,  d)  and  Ca:+  ATPase  models  (e,  I)  showing  the  alkaline  band  under 
calcifying  (c.  e)  and  non-calcifying  conditions  (d.  f).  The  ion  fluxes  detectable  externally  are  highlighted. 


ALGAL  CALCIFICATION 


187 


Smith.  1984a,  b).  The  maximum  pH  in  the  alkaline  band 
would  then  be  about  10.9-1  1.4,  independent  of  solution 
Ca2+  activity. 

For  ATP-driven  2H+/Ca:+  exchange,  the  energy  of  ATP 
hydrolysis  (E)  is  about  -50  to  -55  KJ/mol  (e.g..  Hashi- 
moto el  a/..  1984).  The  electrical  term  in  eq.  (1)  drops 
out,  leaving 


E/2.3RT  = 


-  pCa,)  -  2(pH0  -  pH,)       (3) 


Cytoplasmic  pCa(  =  -log{Ca2+,}  is  about  6.9  (Miller  and 
Sanders,  1987).  Equation  3  describes  a  line  in  (pH(l,  pCa,^ 
space  having  a  slope  of  1/2.  and  displaced  from  the  com- 
position ofthecytosol,  (pH,,  pCa,),  by  4.4  to  4.  8  pH  units. 
The  present  experiments  look  for  evidence  that  CaCO3 
incrustations  accrete  from  the  inside,  as  would  be  expected 
if  the  plant  supplies  the  precipitating  calcium  and  carbon. 
Proton  and  calcium  specific  microelectrodes  search  for 
regions  of  elevated  Ca2+  and  depressed  FT  activities  along 
the  calcifying  surface.  The  combinations  of  calcium  and 
proton  activities  are  compared  with  the  thermodynamic 
constraints  of  ATP  driven  2H+/Ca2+  exchange  against  the 
cytosol.  Calcium  transport  antagonists  are  used  to  inhibit 
proton  uptake.  And  the  stoichiometry  of  calcification  to 
photosynthesis  is  examined  to  see  if  calcium  merely  dif- 
fuses to  the  calcification  site.  In  the  end,  the  Ca2'  ATPase 
model  offers  some  advantages,  but  presents  some  inter- 
esting difficulties.  The  discussion  touches  on  how  the  plant 
uses  calcification  as  a  photosynthetic  adaptation. 

Materials  and  Methods 

The  present  experiments  used  male  plants  of  Cham 
corallina  from  South  Australia,  provided  by  Bill  Lucas. 
Plants  were  maintained  in  the  laboratory,  in  aquaria  ini- 
tially containing  "CPW/B"  solution  (in  mA/,  CaCl2  0.2, 
NaHCO,  1,  NaCl  1,  KG  0.2)  overlying  5-20  cm  mud. 
Nutrients  and  additional  calcium  and  carbon  were  some- 
times added  to  stimulate  growth  and  calcification.  Cool 
white  fluorescent  lights  provided  illumination. 

Regions  of  new  mineralization  were  identified  by  X- 
ray  microanalysis.  Plants  first  accumulated  CaCO,  in  a 
medium  containing  (in  mA/)  CaCl2  2,  NaHCO3  2,  CaSO4 
0.2,  KC1  0.2,  and  NaCl  1,  and  were  then  transferred  to 
media  containing  additional  SrCli  1,  and  MnSO4  0.1.  to 
label  regions  of  new  mineralization.  Cells  showing  heavy 
calcification  and  good  cytoplasmic  streaming  were  rapidly 
frozen  in  liquid  nitrogen  slush,  fractured,  and  given  a  thin 
coating  of  aluminum  by  vacuum  evaporation  (Emscope 
SP2000)  at  -196°C,  to  increase  surface  conductivity. 
Frozen  hydrated  specimens  were  transferred  under  vac- 
uum to  the  cryostage  of  a  scanning  electron  microscope 
(Hitachi  S800)  equipped  with  a  solid  state  X-ray  spec- 
trometer (Kevex  8000  series).  Secondary  electron  mode 
images  provided  details  of  surface  morphology.  X-ray 


maps,  line  scans,  and  area  scans  made  with  an  accelerat- 
ing voltage  of  15  KeV  revealed  distributions  of  Ca,  Sr, 
and  Mn. 

Rates  of  calcification  and  photosynthesis  were  estimated 
from  changes  in  the  alkalinity  and  total  dissolved  inor- 
ganic carbon  content  of  solution.  Alkalinity  was  measured 
by  acidometric  titration  using  the  Gran  method  (Stumm 
and  Morgan,  1970).  Heavily  calcified  plants  were  incu- 
bated in  stoppered  flasks  at  25°C  under  a  mixture  of  flu- 
orescent and  incandescent  lights  for  6-8  h.  Solutions  ini- 
tially contained  (in  m/l//l)  NaHCO,  1,  NaCl  1,  KC1  .2. 
and  0-50  mA/  CaCl,,  pH  8  to  8.2,  adjusted  with  NaOH. 
Calcification  was  calculated  as  half  the  change  in  alkalin- 
ity, and  photosynthesis  was  calculated  as  the  change  in 
total  carbon  minus  calcification. 

In  experiments  designed  to  see  whether  buffered  Ca2+ 
stimulated  photosynthesis  in  solutions  of  low  Ca2+  activ- 
ity, photosynthesis  was  monitored  using  an  oxygen  elec- 
trode (Orion  97-08).  Wide  mouth  jars  (500  ml)  containing 
about  5  g  of  algae  were  filled  with  solutions  prepared  from 
partially  degassed,  deionized  water,  and  capped  under- 
water to  exclude  air  bubbles.  Control  solutions  contained 
(in  mA/)  CaCl:  0.05,  KC1  0. 1 ,  NaCl  1 ,  NaHCO,  1 .8.  and 
Na2CO3  0.2.  Test  solutions  contained  an  additional  0.8 
mA/  CaCl:  and  sodium  citrate  ( 1 .5  mA/).  These  solutions 
exhibited  the  same  Ca2'  activity,  using  a  Ca2+  specific 
microelectrode.  A  relatively  high  pH  (9.1)  ensured  that 
the  plants  obtained  most  of  their  carbon  through  the 
physiology  associated  with  pH  banding.  Half  of  the  plants 
had  been  mostly  decalcified  before  the  experiment  by 
soaking  them  for  2  days  in  a  solution  containing  10  mA/ 
MES  buffer,  initial  pH  5.2.  Cool  white  fluorescent  lights 
provided  illumination  during  2-3  h  incubations  at  about 
25°C. 

The  effect  of  calcium  transport  antagonists  on  inward 
proton  currents  were  investigated  by  exposing  an  illu- 
minated cell  to  LaCl3  or  GdCl3,  while  measuring  proton 
uptake  with  an  extracellular  vibrating  H'  specific  micro- 
electrode  (Kuhtrieber  and  Jaffe,  1990).  The  cell  was 
mounted  in  an  open  Petri  dish  (solution  volume  about  3 
ml)  and  perfused  at  a  rate  of  0.08  ml/s  with  a  solution 
containing,  in  mA/.  CaCl:  0.2,  KC1  0.2,  NaCl  1 .0,  TRIS 
5,  pH  adjusted  to  8.3  with  NaOH.  Fiberoptic  lights  pro- 
vided illumination.  The  proton  electrode  vibrated  per- 
pendicularly to  the  cell  over  an  excursion  of  10  ji,  at  a 
frequency  of  0.5  Hz,  at  a  distance  of  about  10  ^m  from 
the  cell.  4  ^Moles  of  the  lanthanide  was  added  to  the 
input  stream  without  changing  flow  rate.  The  signal  here 
is  the  voltage  difference  registered  by  the  electrode  as  it 
moves  back  and  forth  near  the  cell.  A  proton  gradient  of 
one  pH  unit  within  the  sampled  region  ideally  yields  a 
signal  of  about  58  mV,  although  in  practice  the  signal  is 
smaller.  Fluxes  of  proton  equivalents  carried  by  H+,  OH~, 
and  protonated  TRIS  buffer  were  calculated  from  Pick's 


188 


T.  A.  McCONNAUGHEY  AND  R.  H.  FALK. 


first  law,  using  diffusion  coefficients  93,  53,  and  7  X  10  6 
cnr/s,  respectively,  and  concentrations  calculated  from 
the  measured  pH.  In  the  case  illustrated,  the  pH  at  the 
electrode  was  about  9  before  adding  the  lanthanides.  The 
voltage  field  arising  from  net  charge  uptake  by  the  alkaline 
band  introduces  only  a  small  bias  to  the  pH  signal;  relative 
to  background  solution,  the  alkaline  band  might  show  a 
voltage  differential  of  about  -4  m  V,  while  the  pH  gradient 
of  around  2  units  produces  a  voltage  signal  of  about 
-120  mV. 

Ca2+  and  FT  activities  at  the  alkaline  surface  of  the  cell 
were  measured  using  stationary  ion  specific  microelec- 
trodes,  constructed  as  described  by  Borelli  et  al.  (1985). 
Electrodes  were  connected  to  a  high  impedance  amplifier 
(World  Precision  Instruments  FD223),  with  output  to  a 
chart  recorder.  Additional  potential  sensing  electrodes 
were  sometimes  used  as  well.  The  pericellular  electrical 
field  (about  -4  mV  relative  to  background)  biased  pH 
and  pCa  measurements  by  about  +.07  and  0. 14  pCa  unit, 
respectively.  The  cells  were  exposed  to  buffered  solutions 
of  various  calcium  concentrations,  usually  lacking  dis- 
solved inorganic  carbon  to  discourage  calcification.  Fiber 
optic  lights  provided  illumination. 

In  experiments  comparing  pericellular  pH  against  a 
thermodynamic  model  for  2H+/Ca2+  exchange,  the  pH 
data  represent  the  highest  values  observed  during  electrode 
scans  of  the  cell  surface,  and  during  observations  of  several 
minutes  duration  at  particularly  alkaline  locations.  So- 
lution pCa  was  calculated  from  solution  Ca2+  concentra- 
tions and  ionic  strength,  using  Davies'  individual  ion  ac- 
tivity coefficient  (see  Stum  m  and  Morgan,  1970),  or  mea- 
sured using  an  Orion  93-20  electrode  for  solutions 
containing  citrate.  In  experiments  comparing  simulta- 
neous variations  in  pericellular  pH  and  pCa,  H+  and  Ca:+ 
electrodes  were  placed  close  together  near  the  alkaline 
surface  of  the  cell,  and  the  intensity  of  pH  banding  either 
fluctuated  spontaneously  or  was  modulated  by  turning 
the  light  off  and  on.  In  the  examples  shown,  the  medium 
contained  MOPS  (5  mA/)  and  citrate  (2  mM),  plus  NaOH 
and  CaCl2  to  produce  pH  7.98,  pCa  3.89. 

Extracellular  electrical  currents  were  measured  using  a 
vibrating  probe  electrometer  (Jaffe  and  Nuccitelli,  1974). 
The  probe  vibrated  perpendicular  to  the  cell  surface,  ap- 
proximately 30-50  Mm  from  the  cell  while  the  cell  moved 
by  on  a  motorized  stage.  Fiberoptic  lights  provided  illu- 
mination. Most  experiments  used  nominally  carbon-free 
solutions  containing  (in  mAf)  KC1  0.2,  NaCl  1,  and  a 
zwitterionic  buffer  (MOPS,  PIPES,  EPPS,  CHES,  or 
CAPS,  5  mM),  pH  adjusted  to  the  desired  value  using 
NaOH.  At  the  chosen  concentration  of  CaCl2  (0.1-50 
mM),  the  cell  was  repeatedly  scanned  along  its  length  for 
electrical  activity  while  solutions  of  progressively  higher 
pH  or  Ca2+  concentration  were  added.  The  cell  was  al- 
lowed to  adjust  in  each  solution  for  at  least  30  min.  Elec- 


trical currents  were  calculated  from  the  electrical  con- 
ductivity of  solution,  using  Ohm's  law.  The  example 
shown  used  a  divided  chamber,  so  that  opposite  halves 
of  the  cell  were  exposed  to  different  solutions.  Cytoplasmic 
streaming  between  the  two  halves  was  uninterrupted.  The 
"control"  half  was  bathed  in  CPW/B,  while  the  "test" 
half  went  from  CPW/B  to  carbon-free  solutions  containing 
zwitterionic  buffers  and  20  mM  CaCl2  at  progressively 
higher  pH. 

Results 

Mineralization  patterns 

Calcified  cells  exposed  to  solutions  enriched  in  Sr  and 
Mn  accumulate  significant  Sr  and  Mn  mainly  along  the 
inward  surface  of  CaCO3  incrustations  (Figs.  2.  3.  4).  This 
distribution  suggests  metal  transport  from  the  cell  to  the 
extracellular  site  of  deposition,  although  diffusion  along 
the  cell  wall  is  also  possible.  Mn/Sr  ratios  are  spatially 
variable,  suggesting  some  elemental  segregation  during 
transport  or  precipitation.  This  is  indicated  by  variations 
in  the  relative  intensities  of  their  X-ray  peaks  observed  in 
area  scans.  Some  of  this  variability  is  visible  in  the  X-ray 
maps  presented  in  Figure  3. 


Figure  2.  Scanning  electron  micrograph  of  frozen,  hydrated  cell  la- 
beled with  Sr:+  and  Mn2+.  showing  extracellular  CaCO,  incrustations, 
with  inward  dimpling  of  the  cell  and  apparent  duplication  of  the  cell 
wall.  Magnification:  top  162x,  bottom  830x.  Scale  bar  =  30  ». 


ALGAL  CALCIFICATION  189 

CALCIFICATION  TO  PHOTOSYNTHESIS  RATIO 


15KV  X1.00K    30UM 


Figure  3.  Distributions  of  Ca  (yellow),  Sr  (blue),  and  Mn  (red)  in  an 
extracellular  CaCO3  deposit,  visualized  by  X-ray  mapping  of  a  frozen. 
hydrated  cell  exposed  to  Sr:+  and  Mn:+  after  first  accumulating  significant 
CaCO3. 


Sr  and  Mn  accumulations  presumably  consist  of  di- 
valent metal  carbonates  and  MnO2,  the  latter  inferred 
from  its  dark  color.  Manganese  oxidation,  Mn2+  +  H2O 
+  '/2O2  =  MnO2  +  2H+,  is  favored  in  the  alkaline,  oxygen- 
rich  environment  of  the  plant  surface.  More  or  less  pure 
Mn  accumulations,  based  on  relative  X-ray  counts  for 


120 


4  6 

ENERGY  (KeV) 


10 


Figure  4.  X-ray  spectra  taken  at  points  "A"  and  "B"  of  cell  shown 
in  Figure  2.  Spectra  correspond  to  materials  deposited  after  (A)  and 
before  (B)  addition  of  Sr2+  and  Mn2+  to  the  medium.  X-ray  counts  are 
scaled  relative  to  the  Ca  peak  (100%);  spectrum  A  has  been  shifted  up- 
wards by  20%  for  clarity. 


1  0  - 

D 
D     ,'            *-, 

i 

a'n 

'  °                                          10  - 

°               ~~^j 

0.5 

! 

PHOTOSYNTHESIS 

f  : 

i 

1              05 

1      /' 

0  - 

-j 

-4 

Q_ 

DIC  =  2mM 

pH  -  8                      o  • 

CALCIUM  (mM) 
1234 

J 

10 


20  30 

CALCIUM  (mM) 


40 


50 


Figure  5.  Ratio  of  calcification  to  photosynthesis  near  pH  8  as  a 
function  of  Ca2*  concentration.  Inset:  inhibition  of  photosynthesis  by 
low  Ca2+  concentrations.  Error  bars:  1  S.D. 


Mn,  Sr,  and  Ca,  sometimes  occur  beneath  CaCO,  in- 
crustations, even  when  the  incubating  medium  contains 
considerably  more  Ca2+  and  Sr2+.  Mn  enrichment  may 
reflect  kinetics  of  transport  or  precipitation,  and  was 
probably  assisted  by  oxidation  of  Mn:+  to  Mn4+,  thus 
producing  a  less  soluble,  non-transportable  cation. 

Indentations  of  the  cell  and  apparent  duplications  of 
the  cell  wall  sometimes  occur  underneath  CaCO3  incrus- 
tations (Fig.  2).  Non-calcified  regions  of  the  cell  lack  such 
features.  Calcification  within  the  cell  wall  may  force  the 
plasma  membrane  inward,  followed  by  the  secretion  of  a 
new  wall.  This  scenario  again  suggests  CaCO,  accretion 
to  the  inward  side  of  CaCO3  incrustations,  and  CaCO, 
adhesion  to  the  cell  wall. 

Physiological  stoichiometry 

The  molar  ratio  of  calcification  to  photosynthesis  (C/ 
P),  determined  using  the  pH-alkalinity  method,  is  rela- 
tively constant  at  about  1.0  for  Ca2+  concentrations  be- 
tween 2  and  50  mM  (pH  8,  1  mM  NaHCO3)  (Fig.  5). 
Controls  (dark,  no  algae,  or  boiled  algae)  show  little  cal- 
cification, even  at  50  mM  CaCl2 . 

The  Ca2+  ATPase  model  (Fig.  1 )  correctly  predicts  the 
1:1  C/P  ratio,  provided  the  calcifying  region  is  fairly  iso- 
lated from  bulk  solution.  Each  CO2  precipitated  at  the 
alkaline  band  yields  2H+,  which  the  plant  uses  to  generate 
2CO2  at  the  acid  band.  Calcification  uses  one  CO2,  leaving 
one  for  photosynthesis,  yielding  a  1 : 1  C/P  ratio.  The  pro- 
ton channel  model  would  predict  lower  C/P  ratios,  in- 
creasing with  Ca2+  concentration,  because  a  diffusion 
pathway  must  exist  to  the  calcifying  region.  OH  and 
CO3=  can  therefore  diffuse  away.  These  results  conse- 
quently favor  the  Ca2+  ATPase  model. 


190 


T.  A.  McCONNAUGHEY  AND  R.  H.  FALK. 


Proton  cycling  involves  calcium 

Low  Ca2+  concentrations  inhibit  photosynthesis  (Fig. 
5,  inset).  This  inhibition  appears  to  involve  Ca2+  fluxes, 
because  plants  incubated  at  the  same  low  Ca2+  activity 
show  more  photosynthesis  if  additional  buffered  Ca2+  is 
added  to  solution  (Table  I).  The  rate  of  photosynthesis 
and  the  stimulation  by  buffered  Ca2+  are  greater  with  cal- 
cified than  with  decalcified  plants  (two  way  ANOVA,  both 
factors  and  interactions  significant  at  P  <  0.05). 

Proton  uptake  at  the  alkaline  band,  measured  using  a 
vibrating  FT  specific  electrode,  is  inhibited  by  the  Ca2  + 
transport  antagonists  Gd3+  and  La3+  (Fig.  6).  In  this  ex- 
ample, the  electrode  was  positioned  over  a  point  showing 
particularly  strong  alkalinization.  Gd3+  reduced  the  signal 
registered  by  the  vibrating  electrode  by  about  half,  but 
the  cell  soon  recovered  about  80-90%  of  its  former  signal. 
Subsequent  treatment  with  La3+  reduced  the  signal  more 
strongly,  and  H+  uptake  did  not  recover  for  over  an  hour. 
Before  adding  the  lanthanides,  the  voltage  difference  signal 
registered  by  the  vibrating  electrode  (about  7  mV  at  pH 
9)  corresponds  ideally  to  a  flux  of  proton  equivalents 
around  2  nMoles  cirT2/s,  carried  mostly  by  OH"  and 
TRIS  buffer  (calculation.  Fig.  6  inset). 

The  alkaline  bands  of  C/iara  can  turn  on  and  oft  in- 
dependently, sometimes  without  obvious  provocation,  so 
reductions  in  the  pH  gradient  are  not  necessarily  propor- 
tional to  pathology.  The  pH  gradient  is  also  affected  by 
CaCO}  dissolution  at  the  plant  surface,  and  by  ion  pairing 
and  precipitation  of  the  introduced  lanthanides.  The  ef- 
fects here  appear  to  be  mostly  physiological,  however. 
Perfusion  of  the  chamber  should  have  brought  solution 
pH  back  to  normal  within  a  few  minutes  (theoretical  di- 
lution time  about  38  s). 

An  approximately  2-3  min  oscillation  in  apparent  H  + 
influx  is  observed  in  this  experiment  (Fig.  6).  Such  oscil- 
lations are  detected  using  various  techniques  (Fisahn  et 
ai.  1989),  andean  sometimes  be  induced  by  adding Ca2+ 
to  the  medium. 

Electrochemical  detection  of  calcium  efflux 

Both  the  Ca2+  ATPase  and  proton  channel  models  pre- 
dict Ca2+  diffusion  toward  the  alkaline  surface  under  cal- 

Table  I 

Stimulation  of  photosynthesis  by  buffered  calcium,  at  low  solution 
calcium  activity.  Photosynthesis  estimated  by  oxygen  evolution, 
in  micromoles  O;  per  gram  wet  weight  per  hour, 
with  standard  deviation  (n  =  10) 


> 

HI 

o 


EC 

HI 


U       3  - 
UJ 

O 

|.H 

O 


Solution 

Unbuffered 

Buffered 

Change 

Calcified 
Decalcified 

6.43  ±  0.67 
4.48  ±0.10 

8.11  ±0.68 
5.29  ±0.41 

+26% 
+  18% 

KILOSECONDS 

Figure  6.  Inhibition  of  proton  influx  by  La3t  and  Gd'*,  measured 
with  a  vibrating  proton  specific  electrode.  Ordinate:  voltage  difference 
registered  by  the  electrode  between  the  extremes  of  its  10  micron  excursion 
perpendicular  to  the  cell.  Inset:  apparent  proton  influx  calculated  from 
a  diffusion  model,  as  a  function  of  pH  at  the  probe,  for  a  signal  of  1  mV 
over  an  excursion  of  10  microns.  Fluxes  scale  almost  linearly  with  ex- 
cursion and  voltage. 


diving  conditions.  The  Ca2+  ATPase  model  also  predicts 
localized  Ca:+  efflux,  which,  in  principle,  should  be  de- 
tectable with  Ca2+  specific  microelectrodes.  This  efflux 
might  be  difficult  to  detect,  however.  It  may  occur  un- 
derneath CaCO3  crystals  or  within  an  endomembrane 
system,  and  calcification  may  consume  it  before  it  is  de- 
tected externally.  More  importantly,  Ca2+  influx  and  efflux 
must  both  occur  within  the  alkaline  band  to  produce  its 
electrogenic  character  (Fig.  Ib),  regionally  cancelling  the 
Ca2+  efflux  signal.  Therefore,  detection  requires  a  local 
asymmetry  between  Ca2+  influx  and  efflux  under  non- 
calcifying  conditions  (Fig.  If). 

Such  conditions  encourage  CaCO,  dissolution  and  Ca2+ 
leaching  from  the  cell  wall.  The  resulting  increase  in  peri- 
cellular  Ca2+  concentration  may  be  confused  with  the  ef- 
fects of  2H+/Ca2+  exchange.  The  former  effect  will  be  most 
pronounced  at  low  pH,  while  the  latter  will  be  associated 
with  high  pH.  Simultaneous  pH  observations  are  therefore 
needed  to  distinguish  these  two  cases. 

Increases  in  Ca2+  activity  (pCa  decreases)  are  often  ob- 
served coincident  with  pH  decreases  (Fig.  7a),  suggesting 
Ca2+  leaching  or  CaCO3  dissolution.  Small  drops  in  pCa 
are  also  observed  coincident  with  pH  increases  (Fig.  7b), 
suggesting  2H+/Ca2+  exchange.  At  one  point  in  the  case 
illustrated,  the  apparent  pericellular  Ca2+  activity  increases 
about  30%  as  the  pH  rises  from  8.2  to  9.8.  The  actual 
Ca2+  activity  presumably  increased  even  more,  because 
the  alkaline  band  develops  a  pericellular  electrical  field  of 
around  —4  mV  when  it  turns  on.  This  biases  the  Ca2+ 
electrode  toward  higher  apparent  pCa  by  about  — 4/— 28 
=  0.14  pCa  unit.  The  increase  in  pericellular  Ca2+  due  to 


ALGAL  CALCIFICATION 


191 


40  60 

TIME  (MIN) 

Figure  7.  Extracellular  pH  and  pCa  measured  with  stationary  ion 
specific  electrodes  placed  close  to  the  calcined  surface  of  a  cell  under 
non-calcifying  conditions.  (A)  Positive  correlation  between  pH  and  pCa, 
probably  caused  by  increased  CaCO,  dissolution  or  leaching  at  low  pH. 
(B)  Anticorrelation  between  pH  and  pCa.  suggesting  calcium-proton  ex- 
change. 


2H+/Ca2+  exchange  must  also  be  sufficient  to  overcome 
the  decrease  in  pericellular  Ca2+  at  high  pH.  caused  by 
reductions  in  CaCO,  dissolution  and  Ca2+  leaching  from 
the  cell  wall. 

Thermodynamics 

The  maximum  pH  observed  at  the  alkaline  surface  us- 
ing microelectrodes  approaches  the  thermodynamic  limit 
for  ATP  driven  2H+/Ca:+  exchange,  calculated  using  eq. 
3  (Fig.  8).  The  approach  is  closest  at  high  solution  Ca2+ 
activities  (low  pCa).  As  pCa:+  increases,  the  maximum 
pH  also  increases,  although  not  as  much  as  allowed  by 
thermodynamics.  At  pCa  >4,  higher  pH  readings  are  ob- 
tained in  the  presence  of  the  weak  Ca2+  buffer  citrate, 
suggesting  that  the  rate  of  Ca2+  supply  to  the  cell  may 
limit  proton  uptake.  All  pericellular  pH,  pCa  observations 
fall  within  the  thermodynamic  constraints  for  ATP  driven 
2H+/Ca2+  exchange,  and  the  Ca2+  dependence  for  peri- 
cellular pH  provides  some  support  for  the  Ca2+  ATPase 
model. 

The  pH  and  pCa  in  large  culture  vessels  containing 
Cham  also  approach  the  calculated  thermodynamic  limits 
for  2H+/Ca:+  exchange  (Fig.  8).  The  most  extreme  con- 
ditions observed  (pH  10.78,  pCa  4.30)  are  close  to  the 
most  extreme  conditions  observed  at  the  cell  surface  with 
microelectrodes.  Rather  high  pericellular  pH  (about  10.7) 
is  observed  transiently  in  Ca2+  free  solutions,  but  pH 
banding  eventually  collapses,  consistent  with  a  Ca2+  re- 
quirement for  banding.  Internal  Ca24  stores,  perhaps  sup- 
plemented by  CaCO,  dissolution  and  Ca2+  leaching  from 
the  cell  wall,  may  support  banding  for  awhile. 


Extracellular  electrical  currents 

The  ion  fluxes  associated  with  pH  banding  create  ex- 
tracellular current  loops  which  can  be  measured  with  a 
vibrating  probe  electrometer.  These  currents  persist  until 
solution  pH  is  raised  above  a  critical  value,  at  which  point 
the  currents  cease  or  may  reverse  with  much  diminished 
amplitude.  The  solution  pH  at  which  current  cessation 
occurs  varies  with  solution  Ca2+  activity  in  more  or  less 
the  same  way  as  the  extracellular  pH  data  in  Figure  8. 
The  Ca2+  dependence  suggests  that  proton  uptake  is  cou- 
pled to  Ca2+  expulsion. 

Presumably,  as  2H+/Ca:+  exchange  becomes  impos- 
sible, cytosolic  Ca2+  rises  and  inhibits  Ca2+  influx  (see 
Eckert  and  Chad,  1984).  The  proton  ATPase  of  the  acid 
band  shuts  down  as  the  cytoplasm  becomes  alkalinized 
(due  to  cessation  of  proton  uptake)  and  the  membrane 
potential  increases  (due  to  cessation  of  Ca2+  uptake). 
Consequently,  even  though  2H+/Ca2+  exchange  is  elec- 
trically silent,  preventing  this  exchange  can  stop  extra- 
cellular electrical  activity. 

Figure  9  illustrates  an  experiment  in  which  a  cell  is 
placed  in  a  divided  chamber,  and  increasingly  alkaline 
solutions  containing  20  mAI  CaCN  (pCa  =  2. 1 )  are  applied 
to  the  right  (test)  side.  The  left  (control)  side  remains  at 
pH  8.2,  pCa  =  3.8.  Cytoplasmic  streaming  between  the 
two  sides  is  uninterrupted.  As  the  test  side  approaches  the 
calculated  thermodynamic  limits  for  ATP  driven  2H+/ 
Ca2+  exchange,  its  currents  diminish,  but  currents  on  the 
control  side  are  unaffected.  In  the  last  test  solution  (pH 
10.0,  pCa  2. 1 ).  banding  is  strongly  suppressed  and  an  ap- 


11 


10- 


9- 


pH 


8 


t 
I 

4  » 


CYTOSOL   O 


pH  AT  PLANT  SURFACE 

--  WITH  CITRATE 
CULTURE  SOLUTIONS 


1 


345 

pCa  of  solution 


Figure  8.  pH  observations  at  the  alkaline  surface  as  a  function  of 
solution  Ca2+  activity.  Diagonal  line:  calculated  thermodynamic  limits 
for  ATP  driven  2H+/Ca:+  exchange  between  the  cytosol  and  external 
solution,  assuming  E(  ATP)  =  50  KJ/mol.  Symbols:  (Diamond)  assumed 
cytosolic  composition,  pH  =  8,  pCa  =  6.9.  (Squares)  maximum  pH 
observed  at  alkaline  surface  under  experimental  conditions,  without  ci- 
trate. (  +  )  Same,  with  citrate.  (A)  Combinations  of  solution  pH  and  pCa 
observed  in  large  vat  cultures. 


192 


T.  A.  McCONNAUGHEY  AND  R.  H.  FALK. 


I 

LU 
O 


S3 

Si 


a: 
o 

2 


-1.8 


-20 


-10 


DISTANCE  FROM  DIVIDER  (mm) 


Figure  9.  Extracellular  currents  measured  using  a  vibrating  voltage  probe.  Cell  was  placed  in  a  divided 
chamber,  and  solution  on  the  right  side  was  replaced  with  solutions  having  higher  Ca2*  activity  and  pro- 
gressively higher  pH.  Duplicate  scans  are  shown  in  each  medium,  and  cell  responses  to  different  media  are 
offset  by  -500  /iA/cm2.  Positive  currents  denote  regions  of  positive  current  influx  to  cell  (alkaline  bands). 
Solutions  contained  (in  rruV/)  NaCl  I,  KG  0.2.  plus  the  following  additions:  (a)  CaSO4  0.2,  NaHCO3  1.  pH 
8.2.  (b)  CaCI2  20,  CHES  5,  pH  9.0.  (c)  CaCl,  20.  CAPS  5,  pH  9.8.  (d)  CaCl,  20,  CAPS  5.  pH  10.0). 


parent  efflux  of  positive  charge  prevails  over  the  test  side 
of  the  cell.  The  control  side  does  not  appear  to  compen- 
sate, so  if  real,  this  current  efflux  should  hyperpolarize 
the  cell. 

Discussion 

Antecedents  to  the  Ca2+  ATPase  model  for  Chara  ex- 
tend back  at  least  to  1829,  when  Bishoff  (cited  in  Pring- 
sheim,  1 888)  suggested  that  characean  lime  deposits  grow 
from  the  inside.  Classical  works  on  bicarbonate  use  also 
favored  calcium  and  carbon  movement  through  the  po- 
larized leaves  of  calcareous  aquatic  angiosperms  and 
characeans  to  reach  the  site  of  mineralization  (Arens, 
1933,  1938,  1939).  Kishimoto  et  al.  (1984)  suggested  that 
proton  uptake  in  Chara  might  occur  through  an  electro- 
neutral  proton  cotransport  or  countertransport  system. 
Many  aspects  of  the  Ca2+  ATPase  model  have  therefore 
been  discussed. 

The  Ca2+  ATPase  model  correctly  predicts  the  data 
presented  here.  Sr2+  and  Mn2+  accumulate  largely  along 
the  inner  surface  of  CaCO3  incrustations,  facing  the  cell. 
Increasing  the  Ca2+  concentration  in  solution  (from  2  to 


50  mA/)  has  a  minimal  effect  on  the  ratio  of  calcification 
to  photosynthesis,  suggesting  that  diffusion  to  the  calci- 
fication site  can  be  minimal.  Ca2+  transport  antagonists 
interfere  with  H+  uptake.  In  solutions  of  low  Ca2+  activity, 
additional  "buffered"  Ca:+  enhances  photosynthesis  and 
proton  uptake.  Pericellular  Ca2+  activities  sometimes  in- 
crease simultaneously  with  stronger  alkalization.  Com- 
binations of  extracellular  Ca2+  and  H+  activities  are  ther- 
modynamically  compatible  with  ATP  driven  2H+/Ca2~f 
exchange,  and  the  maximum  pH  at  the  alkaline  band 
increases  with  pCa,  as  would  be  expected  if  Ca2+  extrusion 
accompanies  proton  uptake. 

Most  of  the  precipitating  carbon  also  appears  to  be  sup- 
plied by  the  cell  as  CO2  (McConnaughey,  in  prep.).  This 
further  implies  that  the  calcifying  region  can  become  iso- 
lated from  bulk  solution.  Consequently,  the  cell  must 
supply  Ca2+,  and  remove  protons  in  1:2  stoichiometry, 
as  indicated  by  the  reaction  Ca2+  +  CO2  +  H2O  =  CaCO3 
+  2H+. 

The  data  are  less  supportive  of  the  proton  channel 
model,  which  offers  no  explanation  for  the  Ca2+  depen- 
dence of  photosynthesis,  or  the  elevations  of  pericellular 
Ca2+  coincident  with  H+  depletion.  The  diffusion  pathway 


ALGAL  CALCIFICATION 


193 


to  the  site  of  calcification  creates  additional  conceptual 
problems.  Why  won't  it  accept  OH"  and  CO?=,  which 
should  diffuse  away  from  the  cell,  reducing  the  ratio  of 
calcification  to  photosynthesis  to  values  below  1 .0.  and 
making  it  dependent  on  the  Ca2+  concentration,  or  phos- 
phate, which  fails  to  precipitate  where  Sr  and  Mn  do 
(McConnaughey,  in  prep.)? 

Most  of  the  published  data  appears  compatible  with 
the  Ca2+  ATPase  model.  Both  models  attribute  the  extra- 
cellular current  influx  in  the  alkaline  band  largely  to  Ca:+ 
under  calcifying  conditions,  and  to  H+  equivalents  under 
non-calcifying  conditions  (Fig.  1).  Membrane  hyperpo- 
larizations  are  caused  by  electrogenic  H+  extrusion  in  the 
acid  band  under  either  model.  Increased  membrane  con- 
ductivity at  high  pH  (Bisson  and  Walker,  1980,  1982) 
might  result  from  more  favorable  thermodynamics  for 
the  proton  ATPase  of  the  acid  band,  reversibility  of  2H+/ 
Ca2+  exchange,  and  perhaps  opening  of  additional  ion 
channels  (e.g..  Kikuyama  et  a/.,  1984). 

Calcium  transport 

Certain  caveats  apply  to  the  thermodynamic  analysis 
of  Ca:+  transport  attempted  here.  Pericellular  pCa  varies 
locally,  and  depressions  relative  to  solution  values  are 
likely  at  high  pCa  (Fig.  7).  Cytoplasmic  pH  and  pCa  may 
also  vary.  Extracellular  and  intracellular  activity  scales 
may  be  offset  with  respect  to  each  other.  The  slope  of  the 
extracellular  pH  data  is  closer  to  1/3  than  1/2,  but  pH  is 
too  high  for  ATP  driven  3H+/Ca2+  exchange.  4H+/2Ca:+ 
exchange  is  likewise  excluded.  Several  factors  may  con- 
tribute to  the  fall-off  from  the  limits  calculated  for  2H+/ 
Ca2+  exchange  at  high  pCa.  As  noted  above,  Ca2+  extru- 
sion may  locally  depress  pCa  below  ambient  values.  Ca2+ 
diffusion  toward  the  cell  may  limit  the  rate  of  Ca2+  and 
H+  cycling,  as  suggested  by  the  higher  pH  values  and  pho- 
tosynthetic  rates  obtained  with  buffered  Ca2+.  Finally,  the 
diffusion  of  alkalinity  from  the  plant  surface  increases 
enormously  as  pericellular  pH  increases  (see  Fig.  6  inset), 
so  if  the  proton  flux  remains  constant,  diffusion  should 
reduce  pericellular  pH  most  strongly  at  high  pCa.  In  sum- 
mary, there  are  many  reasons  why  the  data  might  fall 
short  of  the  thermodynamic  limit,  even  if  the  plant  op- 
erates close  to  the  limit.  The  more  interesting  feature  is 
that  the  plant  apparently  approaches  the  thermodynamic 
limit. 

Why  are  extracellular  H+  fluxes  so  much  easier  to  detect 
than  Ca2+  fluxes?  The  difference,  presumably,  is  that  Ca2+ 
influx  and  efflux  occur  close  together,  while  proton  fluxes 
must  be  separated  to  create  the  acid  bands  needed  for 
bicarbonate  assimilation.  Proton  electrodes  are  also  twice 
as  sensitive  as  Ca2+  electrodes,  and  proton  fluxes  should 
be  twice  as  large. 

The  Ca2+  ATPase  model  postulates  high  rates  of  Ca2+ 
cycling  through  the  cell,  around  100  pMol  cm"2  s"1  within 


the  alkaline  band  of  Chara.  45Ca2+  exchange  rates  are 
generally  less  than  3  pMol  cm"2  s"1,  measured  over  the 
whole  cell  (Spanswick  and  Williams,  1965,  Hayama  et 
at.  1979;  MacRobbie  and  Banfield,  1988).  Some  exper- 
iments employed  conditions  unfavorable  to  pH  banding, 
but  the  disparity  between  inferred  and  published  steady 
state  45Ca2+  fluxes  nevertheless  requires  further  study.  Low 
Ca2+  exchange  rates  may  be  caused  by  containment  of 
fluxes  to  the  cortical  cytoplasm  of  the  alkaline  band,  with 
little  exchange  into  major  cellular  Ca2+  reservoirs  such  as 
the  vacuole,  chloroplasts,  or  mitochondria.  The  available 
evidence  supports  this  possibility.  Extracellular  electrical 
currents  presumably  reflect  Ca2+  uptake  mainly  within 
the  alkaline  bands.  When  cells  are  placed  in  a  divided 
chamber  with  Mn:+  on  one  side,  Mn  precipitation  is  vis- 
ible only  on  that  side,  suggesting  minimal  transport  along 
the  cell.  45Ca2+  fluxes  measured  during  repeated  electrical 
stimulation  yield  values  around  60  pMol  cm"2  s"1,  mea- 
sured over  the  whole  cell,  at  1  mAl  external  Ca2+  (Hayama 
el  a!..  1979).  The  metabolic  machinery  needed  for  large 
fluxes  therefore  appears  to  be  present. 

An  analogy  to  coccolithophorid  algae  is  instructive. 
Calcification  in  these  algae  occurs  within  intracellular 
vesicles,  so  both  calcium  and  carbon  presumably  traverse 
the  cytoplasm  to  reach  the  calcification  site.  Ca2+  ATPase 
apparently  participates  in  calcification  (Klaveness,  1976; 
Okazaki  el  a!.,  1984).  Sufficient  data  are  sometimes  avail- 
able to  estimate  Ca2+  fluxes.  For  example,  Emiliania  hux- 
leyi  calcifies  at  a  rate  of  around  5-7  X  10"18  moles/s,  and 
the  surface  area  of  the  finished  coccolith  is  around  1-1.5 
X  10"7  cm2  (Paasche,  1964;  Klaveness,  1976;  Sikes  et  at. 
1980).  Therefore,  the  trans-membrane  Ca2+  flux  may  be 
30-50  pMol  cm"2  s"1.  This  is  of  the  same  magnitude  as 
estimated  for  Chara. 

Because  cytosolic  "free"  Ca2+  concentrations  are  uni- 
formly rather  low,  large  trans-cellular  Ca2+  fluxes  pre- 
sumably involve  Ca2+  rich  vesicles,  vacuoles,  reticula,  etc. 
Total  Ca2+  concentrations  in  characean  cytoplasm  and 
vacuoles  is  in  the  millimolar  range  (Okihara  and  Kiyo- 
sawa,  1988).  To  the  extent  that  cytosolic  free  ion  concen- 
trations pose  a  transport  problem,  the  issue  may  be  more 
acute  with  protons.  The  proton  fluxes  are  presumably 
twice  as  large  and  involve  longer  distances. 

Applicability  to  other  organisms 

If  Ca2+  ATPase  underlies  extracellular  calcification  in 
Chara  and  intracellular  calcification  in  coccolithophorids, 
it  might  contribute  similarly  elsewhere.  For  example,  large 
Ca2+  dependent  proton  influxes,  sensitive  to  lanthanides, 
also  occur  at  the  calcified  rhizoid  of  the  siphonaceous 
marine  alga  Acetabularia  (McConnaughey,  in  prep.). 

Coupling  calcification  to  photosynthesis 

Although  a  photosynthetic  organism  may  loose  CO2  to 
calcification,  it  gains  two  protons  for  each  carbon  lost.  In 


194 


T.  A.  McCONNAUGHEY  AND  R.  H.  FALK 


mildly  alkaline  waters,  these  2H+  potentially  enable  it  to 
convert  2HCO3  to  2CO2,  yielding  a  net  gain  of  one  CO2 
for  photosynthesis.  An  approximately  1:1  ratio  of  calci- 
fication to  photosynthesis  is  observed  not  only  in  Chara, 
but  sometimes  also  in  coccolithophorid  algae  (Paasche, 
1964;  Sikes  el  a/.,  1980),  calcareous  seaweeds  (Pentecost, 
1978),  and  invertebrate-algae  symbioses  (Goreau,  1963; 
Barnes  and  Taylor,  1973;  Duguay  and  Taylor,  1978;  Kuile 
et  ai,  1989).  In  the  symbioses,  the  animal  calcifies  while 
the  algae  use  the  CO;..  Rapid  and  massive  calcification 
may  exceed  structural  or  defensive  uses  for  CaCO3,  and 
coccolithophorids,  for  example,  discard  excess  scales  to 
remain  suspended  in  the  water.  Corals  build  up  huge  skel- 
etal mounds  but  occupy  only  the  top  few  millimeters.  No 
structural  or  defensive  use  of  CaCO3  is  obvious  in  Chara. 

Proton  cycling  theoretically  allows  organisms  to  gen- 
erate pericellular  CO2  concentrations  well  above  ambient 
(Walker  et  ai.  1980).  Photosynthesis  generally  saturates 
at  CO2  concentrations  higher  than  the  atmospheric  equi- 
librium value  (e.g..  Smith  and  Walker,  1980),  and  far 
higher  than  present  in  many  natural  waters  subjected  to 
strong  photosynthesis.  Elevating  CO2  concentrations  (by 
protonating  HCO3  )  therefore  increases  carboxylation 
rate.  Many  aquatic  plants  and  invertebrate-algae  appar- 
ently promote  photosynthesis  through  proton  cycling.  In 
this  context,  protons,  rather  than  CaCO3,  may  be  the 
principle  product  of  calcification. 

From  a  geochemical  perspective,  biologically  precipi- 
tated carbonates  comprise  one  of  the  more  abundant 
crustal  materials,  and  represent  the  principle  biogeo- 
chemical  reservoir  for  carbon  (Garrels  el  ai.  1976).  Be- 
cause organisms  often  calcify  much  faster  than  the  am- 
bient media  in  which  they  live,  biological  calcification 
may  provide  an  important  brake  on  the  photosynthetic 
alkalinization  of  natural  waters,  and  thereby  affect  such 
processes  as  the  partitioning  of  CO2  between  the  oceans 
and  the  atmosphere. 

Acknowledgments 

Primary  funding  was  provided  through  NSF  fellowship 
DCB-88076 1 3  to  Ted  McConnaughey.  W.  J.  Lucas  and 
L.  F.  Jaffe  contributed  laboratory  facilities.  The  authors 
are  grateful  to  J.  Fisahn,  W.  Kiihtreiber,  A.  Miller,  and 
A.  Shipley,  for  their  assistance,  and  to  D.  McCorkle,  A. 
Kuzerian,  C.  Barr,  and  others  for  their  enthusiasm  and 
helpful  comments. 

Literature  Cited 

Arens,  K.  1933.  Physiologisch  polarisierter  Massenaustausch  und  Pho- 
tosynthese  bei  submersen  Wasserpflanzen.  I.  Planla  20:  621-658. 

Arens,  K.  1938.  Manganablagerungen  bei  Wasserpflanzen  als  Folge 
des  Physiologisch  polarisierten  Massenaustausches.  Proloplasma  30: 
104-129. 


Arens,  K.  1939.  Physiologische  Multipolantat  der  zelle  von  Nitella 
wahrend  der  Photosynthese.  Proloplasma  33:  295-300. 

Barnes,  D.  J.,  and  D.  L.  Taylor.  1973.  In  situ  studies  of  calcification 
and  photosynthetic  carbon  fixation  in  the  coral  Monlastrea  anniilaris. 
llelgol.  li'iss.  Meeresunlers.  24:  284-291. 

Bisson,  M.  A.  1984.  Calcium  effects  on  electrogenic  pump  and  passive 
permeability  of  the  plasma  membrane  of  Chara  corallina.  J  Mem- 
brane Biol  81:  59-67. 

Bisson,  M.  A.,  and  N.  A.  Walker.  1980.  The  Chara  plasmalemma  at 
high  pH.  Electrical  measurements  show  rapid  specific  passive  uniport 
of  H  +  or  OH'.  J  Membrane  Biol.  56:  1-7. 

Bisson,  M.  A.,  and  N.  A.  \\alker.  1982.  Control  of  passive  permeability 
in  the  Chara  plasmalemma.  J  E\p  Hot  33:  520-532. 

Borelli,  M.  M.,  W.  G.  Carlini,  W.  C.  Dewey,  and  R.  R.  Ransom.  1985.  A 
simple  method  tor  making  ion-selective  microelectrodes  suitable  for 
intracellular  recording  in  vertebrate  cells.  /  Neurosci.  Methods  15: 
141-154. 

Dixon,  D.  A.,  and  D.  H.  Haynes.  1989.  Ca:+  pumping  ATPase  of  cardiac 
sarcolemma  is  insensitive  to  membrane  potential  produced  by  K+ 
and  Cr  gradients  but  requires  a  source  of  counter-transportable  HT. 
J  Membr  Biol  112:  169-183. 

Duguay,  L.  E.,  and  D.  L.  Taylor.  1978.  Primary  production  and  cal- 
cification by  the  soritid  foraminiferan  Archats  angnlatus  (Fichtel  & 
Moll).  J.  Proloiool.  25:  356-361. 

Eckert,  R.,  and  J.  E.  Chad.  1984.  Inactivation  of  Ca  channels.  Prog. 
Biophys  Molec.  Biol.  44:  215-267. 

Fisahn,  J.,  T.  McConnaughey,  and  \V.  J.  Lucas.  1989.  Oscillations  in 
extracellular  current,  external  pH,  and  membrane  potential  and  con- 
ductance in  the  alkaline  bands  of  Nitella  and  Chara.  J.  E.\p.  Bol.  40: 
1185-1193. 

Garrels,  R.  M.,  A.  Lerman,  and  F.  T.  Mackenzie.  1976.  Controls  of 
atmospheric  CO2  and  O2:  past,  present,  and  future.  Am.  Sci.  64:  306- 
315. 

Goreau,  T.  F.  1963.  Calcium  carbonate  deposition  by  coralline  algae 
and  corals  in  relation  to  their  roles  as  reef-builders.  Ann.  N.  Y  Acad 
Sci.  107:  127-167. 

Hayama,  T.,  T.  Shimmen,  and  M.  Tazawa.  1979.  Participation  of  Ca:+ 
in  cessation  of  cytoplasmic  streaming  induced  by  membrane  exci- 
tation in  Characeae  internodal  cells.  Proloplasma  99:  305-321. 

Jaffe,  L.  F.,  and  R.  Nuccitelli.  1974.  An  ultrasensitive  vibrating  probe 
for  measuring  extracellular  currents.  J.  Cell.  Biol.  63:  614-628. 

Kingsley,  R.  J.,  and  N.  \\atabe.  1985.  Ca-ATPase  localization  and 
inhibition  in  the  gorgonian  Leplogorgia  virgulata  (Lamarck)  (Coe- 
lenterata:  Gorgonacea).  J  Exp.  Mar.  Biol.  Ecol  93:  157-167. 

Kishimoto,  U.,  N.  Kami-iki,  Y.  Takeuchi,  and  T.  Ohdawa.  1984.  A 
kinetic  analysis  of  the  electrogenic  pump  of  Chara  corallina:  I.  In- 
hibition of  the  pump  by  DCCD.  J.  Membrane  Biol.  80:  175-183. 

Klaveness,  D.  1976.  Emiliania  Intxleyi  (Lohman)  Hay  and  Mohler.  3. 
Mineral  deposition  and  the  origin  of  the  matrix  during  coccolith 
formation.  Protistologica  12:  217-224. 

Kuhtreiber,  \V.  M.,  and  L.  F.  Jaffe.  1990.  Detection  of  extracellular 
calcium  gradients  with  a  calcium  specific  vibrating  electrode.  J.  Cell 
Biol  110:  1565-1573. 

Kuile,  B.  ter,  J.  Erez,  and  E.  Padan.  1989.  Mechanisms  for  the  uptake 
of  inorganic  carbon  by  two  species  of  symbiont-beanng  foraminifera. 
Mar.  Biol  103:  241-251. 

Lucas,  \V.  J.  1976.  The  influence  of  Ca:+  and  K/  on  HI4CO3  influx 
in  internodal  cells  of  Chara  corallina.  J.  Exp.  Bol.  27:  32-42. 

Luhring,  H.  and  M.  Tazawa.  1985.  Effect  of  cytoplasmic  Ca2+  on  the 
membrane  potential  and  membrane  resistance  of  Chara  plasma- 
lemma.  Plant  Cell  Physiol.  26:  635-646. 

MacRobbie,  E.  A.  C.,  and  J.  Banfield.  1988.  Calcium  influx  at  the 
plasmalemma  of  Chara  corallina.  Planla  176:  98-108. 


ALGAL  CALCIFICATION 


195 


McConnaughey,  T.  A.  1989a.     Biominerahzation  mechanisms.  Pp.  57- 

73  in  The  Origin.  Evolution,  and  Modem  Aspects  of  Biomineralization 

in  Animals  ami  Plants.  R.  E.  Crick,  ed.  Plenum  Press,  New  York. 
McConnaughey,  T.  A.  1989b.     13C  and  I8O  isotopic  disequilibrium  in 

biological  carbonates.  I.  Patterns.  Geoclum.  Cosiuochnn   Ada  53: 

151-162. 
McConnaughey,  T.  A.  I989c.     "C  and  I8O  isotopic  disequilibrium  in 

biological  carbonates.  II.  In  vitro  simulation  of  kinetic  isotope  effects. 

Geochim.  Cosmoclum.  Ada  53:  163-171. 
Miller,  A.  J.,  and  D.  Sanders.  1987.     Depletion  of  cytosolic  free  calcium 

induced  by  photosynthesis.  Nature  326:  397-400. 
Mimura,  I .,  and  Y.  Kirino.  1984.     Changes  in  cytoplasmic  pH  measured 

by  31P-NMR  in  cells  of  Nilellopxix  ohlusa.  Plant  Cell  Physiol.  25: 

813-820. 
Niggli,  V.,  E.  Sigel,  and  E.  Carafoli.  1982.     The  punned  Ca2+  pump  of 

human  erythrocyte  membranes  catalyzes  an  electroneutral  Ca2+-H+ 

exchange  in  reconstituted  liposomal  systems.  J.  Biol.  Chem   257: 

2350-2356. 
Okazaki.  M.  1977.     Some  enzymatic  properties  of  Ca2+-dependent 

adenosine  tnphosphatase  from  a  calcareous  marine  alga,  Scrralicardia 

maxima  and  its  distribution  in  marine  algae.  But  Mar  20:  347-354. 
Okazaki,  M.,  M.  Fujii,  Y.  Usuda,  and  K.  Furuya.  1984.     Soluble  Ca2+ 

activated  ATPase  and  its  possible  role  in  calcification  in  the  cocco- 

lithophorid  Cricosphaera  roscqffensis  var.  Haptonemofera  (Hapto- 

phyta).  But  Mar  27:  363-369. 
Okazaki,  M.,  and  M.  Tokita.  1988.     Calcification  of  C/iara  hrau/in 

(Charophyta)  caused  by  alkaline  band  formation  coupled  with  pho- 
tosynthesis. Jpn  J  Phycol.  36:  193-201. 
Okihara,  K.,  and  K.  Kiyosawa.  1988.     Ion  composition  of  the  Chara 

mternode.  Plant  Cell  Physiol.  29:  21-25. 
Paasche,  E.  1964.     A  tracer  study  of  the  inorganic  carbon  uptake  during 

coccolith  formation  and  photosynthesis  in  the  coccolithophond  Coc- 

co/ilhus  huxleyi.  Physiol,  Plant  Supp.  III:  5-82. 
Pentecost,  A.  1978.     Calcification  and  photosynthesis  in  corallina  ol- 

hcinalis  L.  using  the  14CO2  method.  Br  Phycol  J  13:  383-390. 
Price,  G.  D.,  and  M.  R.  Badger.  1985.     Inhibition  by  proton  buffers  of 

photosynthetic  utilization  of  bicarbonate  in  Chara  corallina.  Au.il. 

J  Plant  Physio/.  12:  257-267. 
Price,  G.  D.,  M.  R.  Badger,  M.  E.  Bassett,  and  M.  1.  \\hitecross. 

1985.     Involvement  of  plasmalemmasomes  and  carbonic  anhydrase 

in  photosynthelic  utilization  of  bicarbonate  in  Chara  corallina.  Au.il 

J  Plant  Physiol  12:241-256. 
Pringsheim,  N.  1888.     Uber  die  Entstehung  der  Kalkencrustationen  an 

Susswasserpflanzen.  Jahrh.  H'ixxensch.  Bol  19:  138-154. 


Rasi-Caldogno,  F.,  M.  C.  Pugliarello.  and  M.  I.  De  Michelis.  1987.     The 

Ca:+-transport  ATPase  of  plant  plasma  membrane  catalyzes  a  nH+/ 
Ca2+  exchange.  Plant  Physiol  83:  994-1000. 

Raven,  J.  A.,  F.  A.  Smith,  and  N.  A.  Walker.  1986.  Biommeralization 
in  the  Charophyceae  sensu  lain  Pp.  125-139  in  Biomineralization 
in  Lower  Plants  and  Animals.  B.  S.  C.  Leadbeater  and  R.  Riding, 
eds.  Clarendon  Press,  Oxford. 

Sikes,  C.  S.,  R.  D.  Roer,  and  K.  M.  Wilbur.  1980.  Photosynthesis  and 
coccolith  formation:  inorganic  carbon  sources  and  net  inorganic  re- 
action of  deposition.  Lnnnol.  Oceanogr.  25:  248-261. 

Smith,  F.  A.  1984a.  Regulation  of  the  cytoplasmic  pH  of  Chara  cor- 
allina: response  to  changes  in  external  pH.  J.  Ii.\p.  Bol  35:  43-50. 

Smith,  F.  A.  1984b.  Regulation  of  the  cytoplasmic  pH  ol 'Chara  cor- 
allina in  the  absence  of  external  Ca2*:  its  significance  in  relation  to 
activity  and  control  of  the  H+  pump.  J.  Exp.  Bol  35:  1525-1536. 

Smith,  F.  A.,  and  J.  A.  Raven.  1979.  Intracellular  pH  and  its  regulation. 
Ann  Re\:  Plant  Physiol.  30:  289-31 1. 

Smith,  F.  A.,  and  N.  A.  Walker.  1980.  Photosynthesis  by  aquatic  plants: 
effects  of  unstirred  layers  in  relation  to  assimilation  of  CO:  and  HCOr 
and  to  carbon  isotope  discrimination.  AVir  Pliytol.  86:  245-259. 

Spanswick.  R.  M.,  and  A.  G.  Miller.  1977.  Measurement  of  the  cy- 
toplasmic pH  in  \itella  translucens.  Plant  Physiol.  59:  664-666. 

Spanswick,  R.  M.,  and  E.  J.  Williams.  1965.  Ca  fluxes  and  membrane 
potentials  in  Nilella  translucens  J  Exp.  Bol  16:  463-473. 

Spear,  D.  G.,  J.  K.  Barr,  and  C.  E.  Barr.  1969.  Localization  of  hydrogen 
ion  and  chloride  ion  fluxes  in  Nilella.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  54:  397-414. 

Stumm,  \\  .,  and  J.  J.  Morgan.  1970.  At/italic  Chemistry.  Wiley-Inter- 
science.  New  York.  583  pp. 

Tazawa,  M.,  T.  Shimmen,  and  I .  Mimura.  1987.  Membrane  control 
in  the  characeae.  Ann  Re\'  Plant  Physiol.  38:  95-1 17. 

Villalobo,  A.,  and  B.  D.  Roufogalis.  1986.  Proton  countertransport  by 
the  reconstituted  erythrocyte  Ca2+-translocating  ATPase:  evidence 
using  lonophoretic  compounds.  /  Mcmbr.  Biol.  93:  249-258. 

Walker,  N.  A.,  and  F.  A.  Smith.  1977.  Circulating  electrical  currents 
between  acid  and  alkaline  zones  associated  with  HCO,  assimilation 
in  Chara.  J  Exp  Bol  28:  I  190-1206. 

Walker,  N.  A.,  F.  A.  Smith,  and  I.  R.  Cathers.  1980.  Bicarbonate  as- 
similation by  fresh-water  charophytes  and  higher  plants:  I.  Membrane 
transport  of  bicarbonate  ions  is  not  proven.  J.  Membr.  Biol.  57:  51- 
58. 

W  iesenseel,  M.  H.,  and  H.  K.  Ruppert.  1977.  Phytochrome  and  calcium 
ions  are  involved  in  light-induced  membrane  depolarization  in  Nilella. 
Planta  137:  225-229. 


CONTENTS 


BEHAVIOR 

De  Vries,  M.  C.,  D.  Rittschof,  and  R.  B.  Forward  Jr. 

Chemical  mediation  of  larval  release  behaviors  in 

the  crab  Neopanope  sa\i 1 

Hart,  Michael  W. 

Particle  captures  and  the  method  of  suspension 
feeding  by  echinoderm  larvae    12 


DEVELOPMENT  AND  REPRODUCTION 


Patterson,  Mark  R. 

The  effects  of  flow  on  polyp-level  prey  capture  in 

an  octocoral,  Alcyonium  siderium   93 

Purcell,  Jennifer  E.,  Frances  P.  Cresswell,  David  G. 

Cargo,  and  Victor  S.  Kennedy 

Differential  ingest  ion  and  digestion  of  bivalve  larvae 
by  the  scyphozoan  Chrysaora  quinquecirrha  and  the 

ctenophore  Mneiniopu.'*  leidyi 103 

"Walters,  Linda  J.,  and  David  S.  Wethey 

Settlement,  refuges,  and  adult  body  form  in  colonial 
marine  invertebrates:  a  field  experiment    112 


Govind,  C.  K.,  Christine  Gee,  and  Joanne  Pearce 

Retarded  and  mosaic  phenotype  in  regenerated  claw 
closer  muscles  of  juvenile  lobsters  28 

Gustafson,  R.  G.,  D.  T.  J.  Littlewood,  and  R.  A.  Lutz 
Gastropod  egg  capsules  and  their  contents  from 
deep-sea  hydrothermal  vent  environments  34 

Longo,  Frank  J.,  and  John  Scarpa 

Expansion  of  the  sperm  nucleus  and  association  of 
the  maternal  and  paternal  genomes  in  fertilized 
Miilnua  laterals  eggs  56 

Webster,  S.  G.,  and  H.  Dircksen 

Putative  molt-inhibiting  hormone  in  larvae  of  the 
shore  crab  Cumnm  mamas  L.:  an  immunocyto- 
chemical  approach  65 


ECOLOGY  AND  EVOLUTION 

Carlton,  James  T.,  Geerat  J.  Vermeij,  David  R.  Lind- 
berg,  Debby  A.  Carlton,  and  Elizabeth  C.  Dudley 

The  first  historical  extinction  of  a  marine  inverte- 
brate in  an  ocean  basin:  the  demise  of  the  eelgrass 

limpet  Lotlia  alveus s  72 

Patterson,  Mark  R. 

Passive  suspension  feeding  by  an  octocoral  in  plank- 
ton patches:  empirical  test  of  a  mathematical  model        8 1 


PHYSIOLOGY 

Bollner,  Tomas,  Jon  Storm-Mathisen,  and  Ole  Petter 
Ottersen 

GABA-like  immunoreactivity  in  the  nervous  system 
ofOikopleura  diuiin  (Appendicularia)  119 

Charmantier,  G.,  and  M.  Charmantier-Daures 

Ontogeny  of  osmoregulation  and  salinity  tolerance 
in  Cancer  irroratus;  elements  of  comparison  with  C. 
h< 1 1 <•(/ /is  (Crustacea,  Decapoda)  125 

Childress,  J.  J.,  C.  R.  Fisher,  J.  A.  Favuzzi,  R.  E.  Ko- 

chevar,  N.  K.  Sanders,  and  A.  M.  Alayse 

Sulfide-di  iven  autotrophic  balance  in  the  bacterial 
symbiont-containing  hydrothermal  vent  tubeworm, 
Riftiu  (inchyptilti  Jones 135 

Dickson,  John  S.,  Richard  M.  Dillaman,  Robert  D. 

Roer,  and  David  B.  Roye 

Distribution  and  characterization  of  ion  transporting 
and  respiratory  filaments  in  the  gills  of  Procambarus 
flurkii  154 

Dobson,  William  E.,  Stephen  E.  Stancyk,  Lee  Ann 

Clements,  and  Richard  M.  Showman 

Nutrient  translocation  during  early  disc  regenera- 
tion in  the  brittlestar  Microfihiopholis  gracilliina 
(Stimpson)  (Echinodermata:  Ophiuroidea) 167 

McConnaughey,  Ted  A.,  and  Richard  H.  Falk 

Calcium-proton  exchange  during  algal  calcification      185 


Volume  180 


THE 


Number  2 


BIOLOGICAL 
BULLETIN 


LIB 

\ 

APR  171991         « 


Hole, 


APRIL,  1991 


Published  by  the  Marine  Biological  Laboratory 


THE 

BIOLOGICAL  BULLETIN 


PUBLISHED  BY 
THE  MARINE  BIOLOGICAL  LABORATORY 


Associate  Editors 


Marine  Biological  Laboratory 
LIBRARY 


APR  1?  1991 


Woods  Hole,  Mass. 


PETER  A.  V.  ANDERSON,  The  Whitney  Laboratory,  University  of  Florida 

DAVID  EPEL,  Hopkins  Marine  Station,  Stanford  University 

J.  MALCOLM  SHICK.,  University  of  Maine,  Orono 

Editorial  Board 

GEORGE  J.  AUGUSTINE,  University  of  Southern  RUDOLF  A.  RAFF,  Indiana  University 

California 

KENSAL  VAN  HOLDE,  Oregon  State  LJniversity 

Louis  LEIBOVITZ,  Marine  Biological  Laboratory  STEVEN  VOGEL,  Duke  University 


Eclitur:  MICHAEL  J.  GREENBERG,  The  Whitney  Laboratory,  University  of  Florida 
Managing  Editor.  PAMELA  L.  CLAPP,  Marine  Biological  Laboratory 


APRIL,  1991 

Printed  and  Issued  by 
LANCASTER  PRESS,  Inc. 

PRINCE  &  LEMON  STS. 
LANCASTER,  PA 


Erratum 

The  Biological  Bulletin,  Volume  179,  Number  3,  pages  358  and  363 

The  following  corrections  should  be  made  in  the  article  by  William  J.  Kuhns  et  al.  titled,  "Biochemical 
and  functional  effects  of  sulfate  restriction  in  the  marine  sponge,  Microciona  prolifera"  (Biol.  Bull.  179: 
358-365).  Due  to  a  printing  error,  the  last  two  lines  of  the  abstract  on  page  358  were  transposed  from  the 
first  column  (abstract)  to  the  second  column  (introduction).  We  apologize  for  the  error. 

On  page  363,  the  sentence  beginning  on  line  7  of  Figure  5  should  now  read  "The  upper  two  lines  depict 
uptake  of  35SO4  by  cells  pretreated  in  MBL-SO4."  The  words  "upper  two  lines"  replace  the  words  "solid 
lines." 


Reference:  Biol.  Bull  180:  197-199.  (April,  1991) 


Integrative  Neurobiology  and  Behavior  of  Mollusks 

Symposium:  Introduction,  Perspectives,  and 

Round-Table  Discussion 


ROGER  T.  HANLON 

The  Marine  Biomedical  Institute.  University  of  Texas  Medical  Branch, 
Galveston,  Texas  77550 


The  objective  of  this  symposium  was  to  bring  together 
molluscan  researchers  from  a  wide  variety  of  disciplines 
to  consider  the  behavior  of  mollusks,  particularly  in  re- 
lation to  their  evolutionary  history  and  to  their  neural 
structure  and  function.  For  that  reason,  the  symposium 
was  convened  during  the  56th  Annual  Meeting  of  the 
American  Malacological  Union  (AMU),  attended  by  280 
persons  and  held  at  the  Marine  Biological  Laboratory 
(MBL)  in  Woods  Hole  from  3-7  June  1990.  The  AMU 
membership  includes  many  researchers  interested  pri- 
marily in  systematics,  phylogeny.  and  evolution:  the  MBL 
enjoys  a  long  and  distinguished  history  as  a  global  center 
for  molluscan  neurobiological  research.  Thirty-two  papers 
and  seven  posters  were  presented  at  this  symposium, 
which  was  held  concurrently  with  The  Behavior  of  Mol- 
lusks symposium  in  which  26  papers  and  1 2  posters  were 
presented;  selected  papers  from  the  latter  symposium  will 
be  published  in  the  American  Malacological  Bulletin  dur- 
ing 1991.  Support  for  the  symposia  was  provided  by  the 
National  Science  Foundation  (BNS  9007661)  and  the 
AMU  Symposium  Endowment  Fund.  The  organizers  and 
participants  greatly  appreciate  this  funding  as  well  as  the 
assistance  of  The  Biological  Bulletin  and  the  hospitality 
of  the  MBL. 

Perspectives 

The  Phylum  Mollusca  is  large  (about  100,000  liv- 
ing species),  diverse,  well  represented  in  the  fossil  rec- 
ord (approx.  35,000  species  dating  to  the  Cambrian),  and 
richly  studied  (cf.,  Tasch,  1973;  Wilbur,  1983-1988; 
Barnes,  1987).  It  is  also  important  to  man;  its  uses  range 
from  food  to  models  in  biomedicine.  In  the  latter  instance 


it  has  been  mainly  species  with  unusually  large  or  easily 
identified  neurons  that  have  been  studied  so  intensively. 
These  species  were  well  represented  in  the  symposium: 
e.g..  Aplysia,  Hermissenda,  Navanax,  Pleurobranchaea. 
Tritonia.  Lymnaea,  and  Loligo  among  several  others. 

Molluscan  diversity  offers  fertile  grounds  for  thought 
among  evolutionary  biologists,  ethologists,  and  neuro- 
biologists.  In  what  other  phylum  can  you  find  organisms 
as  different  as  chitons,  with  their  simple  nervous  system 
and  behavior,  and  cephalopods,  with  their  immensely 
complex  nervous  system  and  correspondingly  complex 
and  varied  behavior  that  rival  vertebrates?  In  the  middle 
of  this  continuum,  consider  the  marine  opisthobranchiate 
gastropod  Aplysia  and  some  of  its  cousins,  whose  relatively 
simple  behaviors  are  being  studied  even  at  molecular 
levels  by  thousands  of  researchers  worldwide.  The  chal- 
lenge, of  course,  is  to  make  some  sense  of  this  dazzling 
diversity. 

The  scope  and  goals  of  the  relatively  new  field  of 
Neuroethology  have  been  well  reviewed  by  Hoyle 
(1984)  and  Bullock  (1990).  As  Bullock  (1990)  explains 
eloquently,  although  neurobiologists  mainly  study  prox- 
imate mechanisms  of  neural  function,  the  implications 
of  their  findings  are  basic  to  the  philosophy  of  science  and 
to  evolutionary  biology  because  it  is  likely  that  the  nervous 
system  and  behavior  represent  the  system  most  responsible 
for  large  evolutionary  leaps  in  the  grade  of  complexity 
among  higher  taxa.  Notwithstanding  this  provocative  as- 
sertion, most  neurobiologists  do  not  spend  much  time 
mingling  with  evolutionary  biologists,  especially  those  who 
study  the  same  phylum,  because  their  professional  orga- 
nizations and  journals  are  usually  quite  distinct.  Con- 
versely, evolutionary  biologists  do  not  often  consider 


197 


198 


R.  T.  HANLON 


proximate  mechanisms  of  neural  control  of  behavior  or 
its  implications  in  evolution.  This  meeting  and  its  round- 
table  discussion  were  organized  to  help  bridge  this  gap 
among  molluscan  researchers. 

Round-Table  Discussion 

Are  round-table  discussions  worthwhile?  While  con- 
troversial, they  are  worthwhile  in  forcing  the  consider- 
ation of  differing  views  and  alternative  hypotheses 
in  a  less  binding  way  than  a  paper  or  poster  presentation. 
I  include  here  the  briefest  synopsis  of  our  recorded  3-h 
session. 

Can  the  comparative  method  bridge  studies  involving 
neurobiology,  behavior,  and  evolution? 

There  was  some  consensus  that  proper  use  of  homol- 
ogies  could  provide  common  ground  to  test  hypotheses 
across  these  disciplines.  However,  gaps  in  our  knowledge 
in  each  broad  discipline  are  considerable,  and  future 
progress  was  anticipated  to  be  slow  unless  workers  were 
willing  to  take  evidence  from  many  disciplines  (including 
molecular  and  neural  biology,  behavior  and  ecology)  and 
integrate  it  into  analyses  of  convergence  and  parallelism 
in  the  Mollusca.  This  was  one  of  the  few  points  of  general 
consensus! 

Are  there  any  uniquely  molluscan  behaviors? 

As  a  matter  of  perspective,  only  a  dozen  or  so  species 
are  being  studied  in  detail,  so  generalizations  about  mol- 
luscan behavior  seem  inappropriate;  no  unique  behaviors 
were  mentioned.  It  was  noted  that  some  groups  of  mol- 
lusks  have  similar  nervous  system  organization  but  ap- 
parently very  different  behaviors. 

What  is  the  role  of  behavior  in  evolution? 

Some  researchers  consider  behavior  to  be  the  phenotype 
upon  which  selection  occurs,  and  the  corollary  is  that 
the  neural,  endocrine,  and  related  systems  exist  to 
produce  behavior.  Perhaps,  then,  homologies  at  differ- 
ent levels  of  organization  can  be  used  to  study  the  role 
of  behavior  in  evolution.  However,  several  concerns 
were  voiced  about  the  seemingly  endless  variability  of 
molluscan  behavior  that  would  make  it  difficult  to  discern 
homologs  in  any  concrete  fashion.  How,  for  example,  do 
we  account  for  phenotypic  plasticity,  and  what  is  the  role 
of  ecological  constraint?  One  related  subject  of  interest  to 
many  in  attendance  was  the  future  need  for  analyses  of 
the  evolution  of  behavior  in  mollusks,  taking  advantage 
of  what  has  been  learned  from  paleobiology  and  taphon- 
omy  (Tasch,  1973)  as  well  as  recent  behavioral  and  eco- 
logical studies  of  extant  molluscs. 


How  did  the  nervous  system  evolve? 

The  action  potential  is  a  common  feature  throughout 
the  animal  phyla,  from  the  simplest  organisms  to  the  most 
complex.  However,  pharmacological  sensitivities  and 
other  properties  are  different  in  various  groups  and  it  was 
suggested  that  molecular  analysis  of  specific  differences 
in  the  relevant  ion  channels  may  eventually  lead  to  mean- 
ingful phylogenetic  relationships  among  different  phyla. 
The  role  of  peptides  and  other  neurotransmitters  in  pro- 
ducing behavior  (e.g.,  egg  laying  in  many  mollusks)  may 
lead  to  complementary  findings  (see  recent  papers  in  Bio- 
logical Bulletin  Vol.  177,  1989). 

\\'hv  do  we  find  large  neurons  in  some  mollusks,  and 
what  is  their  functional  and  evolutionary  significance? 

One  obvious  function  for  large  cells  is  the  rapid  trans- 
mission of  a  signal;  additionally,  cells  must  be  large  if  they 
have  to  spread  over  large  distances  to  communicate  with 
many  places.  If  both  needs  must  be  satisfied,  giant  cells 
like  the  squid  axon  can  obviously  evolve.  Perhaps  many 
of  these  cells  are  associated  with  relatively  "simple"  be- 
haviors. There  does  seem  to  be  an  emerging  trend  that 
small  neuron  size  is  a  requirement  for  complex  infor- 
mation processing.  For  example,  consider  the  cephalopod 
CNS  and  its  complex  integrative  abilities  in  comparison 
with  the  vertebrate,  and  especially  the  mammalian,  CNS. 
It  is  apparent  that  existing  and  future  neurobiological 
techniques  will  dictate  to  some  extent  what  can  be  studied 
and  learned  from  small  versus  large  neurons.  There  will 
probably  continue  to  be  philosophical  disagreement  about 
the  reductionist  versus  the  integrative  approach  to  un- 
derstanding how  behavior  is  produced  by  the  nervous 
system. 

A  thread  throughout  the  discussion  was  the  amazing 
diversity  of  mollusks  and  the  difficulty  in  agreeing  on 
many  generalizations  about  neural  organization  and  how 
it  relates  to  behavior.  It  was  suggested  several  times  that 
we  should  appreciate  this  diversity  and  not  rush  to  apply 
our  findings  to  evolutionary  principles.  As  Bullock  ( 1 990) 
has  pointed  out  "Must  we  assume  everything  is  adaptive?" 
Various  participants  echoed  a  recent  thought  that  evo- 
lution does  not  necessarily  work  with  the  logic  of  engi- 
neering, but  perhaps  more  like  a  tinkerer.  The  papers  in 
this  volume  include  many  examples  of  neurobiologists 
evaluating  evolutionary  considerations  of  the  systems  that 
they  study.  Perhaps  the  discussions  facilitated  this  in  some 
small  way. 

Dedication 

We  were  honored  to  have  Professor  J.  Z.  Young,  F.R.S., 
of  Oxford  University,  enrich  our  meeting.  In  the  1930s, 


INTRODUCTION 


199 


Figure  1 .     Professor  J.  Z.  Young  presenting  his  review  ol  the  cellular 
basis  of  learning  in  eephalopods. 


Professor  Young  rediscovered  the  squid  giant  axon  and 
performed  the  critical  experiments  demonstrating  that 
these  neurons  transmitted  an  electrical  signal;  this  pi- 
oneering work  (performed  partly  at  the  MBL)  led  to  the 
Nobel  Prize  by  Hodgkin  and  Huxley  in  1962  and  made 
the  squid  Loligo  a  preferred  research  organism  for  thou- 
sands of  neuroscientists,  a  trend  that  continues  apace  to- 
day. In  addition  to  recounting  this  story  at  the  banquet. 
Professor  Young  gave  a  rousing  lecture  on  learning  in 
eephalopods  (Fig.  1 ).  Delivered  with  panache  and  his  usual 
scientific  fervor,  he  earned  a  standing  ovation  by  a  packed 
audience  in  Whitman  Auditorium.  For  his  innumerable 
contributions  to  molluscan  biology.  Professor  Young 
was  awarded  Honorary  Life  Membership  in  the  AMU, 
a  distinction  bestowed  upon  only  five  members  in 
the  56-year  history  of  the  organization.  We  hereby  dedi- 
cate this  volume  to  him  and  thank  him  for  inspiring  so 
many  students  and  researchers  throughout  his  brilliant 
career. 

Literature  Cited 

Barnes,  Robert  D.  1987.  Invcrlchralc  /.oology.  Fifth  Edition.  CBS  Col- 
lege Publishing/Holt.  Rinehart  and  Winston,  Philadelphia,  PA. 

Bullock.  Theodore  Holmes.  1990.  Goals  of  neuroethology.  Bioscience 
40(4):  244-248. 

Hoyle,  Graham.  198-4.  The  scope  of  neuroethology.  Behav.  Brain  Sci. 
7:367-412. 

Tasch,  Paul.  197.1.  Palenbiiilugy  of  the  Invertebrates:  Data  Retrieval 
from  Ike  Fossil  Record.  Wiley  and  Sons.  New  York. 

Wilbur,  Karl  M.  1983-1988.  The  Mollusca.  Volumes  1-12.  Academic 
Press.  New  York. 


Reference:  Bio/.  Bull.  180:  200-208.  (April, 


Computation  in  the  Learning  System  of  Cephalopods 


J.  Z.  YOUNG 

Department  of  Experimental  Psychology,  University  of  Oxford, 
South  Parks  Road,  Oxford  OX1  3UD,  United  Kingdom 


Abstract.  The  memory  mechanisms  of  cephalopods 
consist  of  a  series  of  matrices  of  intersecting  axes,  which 
find  associations  between  the  signals  of  input  events  and 
their  consequences.  The  tactile  memory  is  distributed 
among  eight  such  matrices,  and  there  is  also  some  suboe- 
sophageal  learning  capacity.  The  visual  memory  lies  in 
the  optic  lobe  and  four  matrices,  with  some  re-exciting 
pathways.  In  both  systems,  damage  to  any  part  reduces 
proportionally  the  effectiveness  of  the  whole  memory. 
These  matrices  are  somewhat  like  those  in  mammals,  for 
instance  those  in  the  hippocampus. 

The  first  matrix  in  both  visual  and  tactile  systems  re- 
ceives signals  of  vision  and  taste,  and  its  output  serves  to 
increase  the  tendency  to  attack  or  to  take  with  the  arms. 
The  second  matrix  provides  for  the  correlation  of  groups 
of  signals  on  its  neurons,  which  pass  signals  to  the  third 
matrix.  Here  large  cells  find  clusters  in  the  sets  of  signals. 
Their  output  re-excites  those  of  the  first  lobe,  unless  pain 
occurs.  In  that  case,  this  set  of  cells  provides  a  record  that 
ensures  retreat. 

There  is  experimental  evidence  that  these  distributed 
memory  systems  allow  for  the  identification  of  categories 
of  visual  and  tactile  inputs,  for  generalization,  and  for 
decision  on  appropriate  behavior  in  the  light  of  experience. 

The  evidence  suggests  that  learning  in  cephalopods  is 
not  localized  to  certain  layers  or  "grandmother  cells"  but 
is  distributed  with  high  redundance  in  serial  networks, 
with  recurrent  circuits. 

Introduction 

Responding  appropriately  in  a  complex  environment 
depends  upon  the  categorization  of  events  and  a  decision 
of  what  to  do.  Animals  with  good  brains  have  the  ability 
to  learn  the  useful  responses  to  particular  events  that  they 
encounter.  They  may  not  be  born  with  receptors  tuned 

Received  7  August  1990;  accepted  18  January  1991. 


to  identify  objects  or  situations,  say  a  rock  or  a  tree  or  a 
fish,  but  learn  the  classification  of  particular  sets  of  stimuli 
by  virtue  of  the  large  number  of  their  neurons.  It  has  been 
claimed  that  this  involves  simply  "the  spontaneous  emer- 
gence of  new  computational  capabilities  from  the  collec- 
tive behaviour  of  large  numbers  of  simple  processing  ele- 
ments" (Hopfield.  1982).  Biologists  will  probably  suspect 
that  a  genetic  component  is  involved  in  the  organization. 

Cephalopods  have  such  nervous  systems  with  numerous 
neurons,  and  there  is  sufficient  information  about  their 
arrangement  to  suggest  how  they  function.  Formerly,  I 
have  emphasized  that  the  circuits  in  their  brains  must 
allow  for  the  outputs  from  feature  detectors  to  produce 
alternative  effects  after  learning  (Fig.  1).  I  proposed  that 
the  feature  detectors  must  become  restricted  during 
learning  to  establish  units  of  memory  or  mnemons.  This 
view  is  correct  in  that  it  emphasizes  the  possibility  of  al- 
ternative outputs  from  feature  detectors,  but  it  is  much 
too  restrictive.  Emphasis  on  units  obscures  the  essential 
fact  that  these  are  systems  with  numerous  parallel,  inter- 
acting channels.  It  is  now  evident  that  the  various  lobes 
of  the  brain  provide  sequences  of  matrices  of  intersecting 
axes,  with  feedback.  They  enable  the  identification  of  cat- 
egories of  input  and  storage  of  records  of  the  probable 
value  of  each,  in  the  form  of  bias  to  particular  directions 
of  action  to  each  set  of  input  signals. 

The  principle  of  the  matrices  is  to  provide  for  selection 
of  paths  that  are  used  and  inhibition  of  those  that  are  not 
used.  This  is  accomplished  by  various  means  that  allow 
interaction  between  pathways.  One  of  the  best  analyzed 
systems  is  in  the  mammalian  hippocampus  (Fig.  2)  (Rolls, 
1990).  In  a  competitive  learning  matrix  such  as  the  dentate 
gyrus,  "different  input  patterns  on  the  horizontal  axons 
will  tend  to  activate  different  output  neurons.  The  ten- 
dency for  each  pattern  to  select  different  neurons  can  be 
enhanced  by  providing  inhibition  between  the  output 
neurons.  .  .  .  Synaptic  modification  then  occurs  .  .  .  and 


200 


COMPUTATION  IN  CEPHALOPOD  LEARNING  SYSTEM 


201 


Feature 
detectors 


Basal 
lobes 


Attack 
Taste          Medsupfr 


Vertical 


Subvert  ic  al 


Magno 
cellular 


Retreat 


Figure  1.  Scheme  to  show  alternative  pathways  from  the  visual  fea- 
ture detectors  of  an  octopus.  There  are  output  pathways  for  attack  or 
retreat.  A  third  pathway  leads  to  the  four  matrices  of  the  vertical  lobe 
system.  Here  particular  patterns  of  visual  signals  are  combined  with  those 
of  taste  to  increase  the  future  tendency  to  attack,  or  with  signals  of  pain 
to  reduce  it. 


the  response  of  the  system  as  a  categorizer  climbs  over 
repeated  iterations"  (Rolls,  1990). 

In  autocorrelation  networks,  such  as  the  CAi  cells 
(Fig.  2),  the  preferred  pathways  are  reinforced  by  mutu- 
ally strengthening  each  other.  In  this  case,  the  cells  of 
the  matrix  have  collaterals  that  feed  back  to  their  own 
inputs.  These  recurrent  synapses  follow  the  Hebb  rule 
so  that  "any  strongly  activated  cell  or  set  of  cells 


becomes  linked  by  strengthened  synapses  with  any  other 
conjunctively  activated  cell  or  set  of  cells"  (Rolls,  1990). 
As  a  result,  during  recall  "presentation  of  even  part  of  the 
original  pattern  .  .  .  comes  to  elicit  the  firing  of  the 
whole  set  of  cells  that  were  originally  conjunctively 
activated." 

In  the  hippocampus,  this  result  is  achieved  by  the  col- 
laterals of  the  CA3  cells,  which  reactivate  the  dendrites  of 
their  own  and  a  large  number  of  other  CA,  cells.  It  may 
be  that  in  cephalopods  a  similar  effect  is  achieved  by  pass- 
ing the  signals  through  a  series  of  lobes,  each  serving  as  a 
matrix  whose  output  may  be  returned  to  a  previous 
member  of  the  series  (Fig.  3).  The  functioning  of  any  such 
matrix  system  depends  on  the  particular  anatomical  ar- 
rangements and  details  of  synaptic  functioning  and  its 
alteration  with  use.  We  do  not  know  enough  about  such 
factors  in  cephalopods  to  be  able  to  specify  precisely  how 
they  operate.  However,  the  system  is  simple  enough  to 
allow  us  to  follow  the  whole  sequence  through  these  lobes, 
from  sense  organs  to  motor  output,  and  at  least  to  spec- 
ulate about  its  functioning. 

Matrix  systems  of  this  sort  have  the  properties  that  we 
associate  with  complex  animal  behavior.  They  ensure  that 
there  is  generalization:  presentation  of  even  a  part  of  the 
original  figure,  or  one  like  it,  activates  the  firing  of  the 
whole  set  of  cells  that  were  conjunctively  activated 
("completion"").  Moreover,  the  system  continues  to  operate 
even  if  some  of  the  cells  fail  to  operate  or  are  removed 
("fault  tolerance").  We  can  show  that  these  essential 


FDgc 


CA1 


Neoc  or  te  x 


Parahippocarripal 
gyrus 


to   septum 
mammillary   bodies 


Parahippccampal 
gyrus 

1          1 

Neocorte  x 


Figure  2.  Schematic  representation  of  the  scheme  of  matrices  and  connections  within  the  primate  hip- 
pocampus and  with  the  neocortex.  The  competitive  matrix  in  the  dentate  gyrus  leads  to  an  auto  association 
matrix  formed  by  the  CA3  cells,  which  in  turn  lead  to  a  competitive  matrix  on  the  CA,  cells  (From  Rolls, 
1990,  with  permission). 


202 


J.  Z.  YOUNG 


median 

inferior  frontal 
>2 


sub 
frontal 
3  ,v 


trauma 


trauma 


'sucker 


Figure  3.  Diagram  of  the  connections  of  the  tactile  memory  system 
ofOclopus.  The  successive  matrices  are  labelled  1  to  8.  In  addition,  there 
is  some  learning  capacity  in  the  suboesophageal  centers. 


properties  of  memory  systems  are  present  in  the  nervous 
system  of  octopuses. 

The  nervous  system  of  an  octopus  provides  for  cate- 
gorization and  setting  up  of  memories  both  for  vision  and 
touch.  There  are  two  distinct  systems,  each  made  up  of 
four  lobes,  with  elements  whose  arrangement  can  now  be 
seen  to  constitute  sequences  of  matrices  (Fig.  3,  4).  It  was 
thought  at  one  time  that  these  were  independent  visual 
and  tactile  systems,  but  it  is  now  clear  that  in  tactile  learn- 
ing all  eight  lobes  are  involved  (Young,  1983).  This, 
therefore,  constitutes  a  remarkable  example  of  a  distrib- 
uted memory  system  using  a  series  of  networks.  This 
model  is  rather  similar  to  that  suggested  by  Wells  (1978). 
It  does  not  depend  on  detailed  preformed  connections 
and  so  avoids  problems  of  complex  morphogenesis. 

The  Chemo-Tactile  Memory  System 

Octopuses  readily  recognize  differences  in  the  chemical 
nature  and  texture  of  objects  by  touch,  although  they  can- 
not discriminate  between  shapes  (Wells,  1978).  The  re- 
ceptors for  touch  are  in  the  rims  of  the  suckers  (see  Gra- 
ziadei  in  Young,  1971).  Their  axons  proceed  through 
synapses  in  the  arm,  but  no  details  are  known  of  the  coding 
signals  that  are  sent  to  the  brain. 

In  our  experiments.  Wells  and  I  train  octopuses  to  dis- 
tinguish between  plastic  balls,  either  smooth  or  with  up 
to  thirteen  incised  rings  (Wells,  1978;  Young,  1983).  We 
train  an  animal  by  giving  it  food  when  it  takes  one  ball 
(say  a  smooth  one)  and  by  giving  it  no  reward  or  a  small 
electric  shock  for  taking  the  other  (rough)  (Fig.  5).  In  crit- 
ical experiments,  the  optic  nerves  are  cut  to  avoid  possible 


visual  discrimination.  Many  of  the  experiments  are  done 
after  the  whole  supraoesophageal  lobe  has  been  bisected. 
The  arms  of  the  two  sides  then  learn  independently  and 
can  even  be  trained  in  opposite  directions. 

Afferent  fibers  from  the  arms  and  also  taste  fibers  from 
the  lips  cross  the  dendrite  systems  of  the  first  tactile  lobe, 
the  lateral  inferior  frontal  (Fig.  6,  7).  The  axons  of  the 
cells  of  this  first  lobe  pass  partly  to  the  fourth  lobe,  the 
posterior  buccal,  and  partly  to  the  lateral  superior  frontal 
and  so  to  the  vertical  lobe  system  (below).  The  fibers  from 
the  arms  and  lips  then  pass  on  to  the  second  matrix,  in 
the  median  inferior  frontal,  where  they  interweave  and 
cross  the  trunks  of  a  large  sample  of  the  10"  cells,  this 
allows  maximum  opportunity  for  any  cell  of  the  lobe  to 
receive  signals  from  a  variety  of  input  fibers  (Fig.  7).  These 
median  inferior  frontal  cells  then  send  their  axons  to  the 
third  matrix,  the  subfrontal  lobe,  which  contains  relatively 
few  large  cells  with  twisted  trunks  and  many  bushy  den- 
drites  and,  in  addition,  a  great  number  (5  X  10b)  of  very 
small  amacrine  cells.  The  subfrontal  also  receives  nu- 
merous fibers  from  below,  presumed  to  signal  trauma. 
The  large  subfrontal  cells  send  their  axons  to  the  fourth 
lobe,  the  posterior  buccal,  from  which,  in  turn,  large  axons 
pass  directly  to  the  arms  and  cause  them  either  to  draw 
in  or  reject  the  object  touched  (Budelmann  and  Young, 
1985).  These  cells  must  be  of  two  sets,  some  causing  the 
object  touched  to  be  drawn  in,  the  others  to  reject  it. 


subv 


med^upfr        medinf.fr 


ant  has 


sub.fr 


sup.  buc 


Figure  4.  Sagittal  section  of  the  supraoesophageal  lobe  of  Octopus 
mlgaris  stained  with  Cajal's  silver  method.  Abbreviations  for  all  figures: 
ant.  bas.,  anterior  basal;  b.  med.,  median  basal;  buc.  p.,  posterior  buccal; 
cer.  br.  con.,  cerebrobrachial  connective;  cer.  tr..  cerebral  tract;  lat.  int. 
fr,  lateral  inferior  frontal;  lat.  sup.  fr.,  lateral  superior  frontal;  mag., 
magnocellular;  med.  inf.  fr..  median  inferior  frontal;  med.  sup.  fr.,  median 
superior  frontal;  op.,  optic;  ped.,  peduncle;  post,  buc.,  posterior  buccal; 
plex.,  plexiform  layer,  prec.,  precommissural:  pv.,  palliovisceral;  ret., 
retina;  subfr..  subfrontal;  sup.  buc..  superior  buccal;  subv..  subvertical; 
vert.,  vertical. 


COMPUTATION  IN  CEPHALOPOD  LEARNING  SYSTEM 


203 


4                                           tests                             tests  -, 

24 

smooth  +  fish               1       smooth  +  fish            1 

i£ 

-/ 

"5 

73 
»_* 

(M 

33 

*™ 

v».-'*'^ 

CJ 

c 

rough,       "~\ 

C 

v 
M 

no  reward 

5  2 

—                                                             V-* 

12  _^ 

>v 

w 

-5 

.-*-,      rough 
x-,+  shock 

jj 

03 

\ 

E 

V-*., 

'v_ 

11+12                              21+2'_ 

1                                                                                      t 

n 

!      !      I      1      1      1      1      1      1      II     1      1      !      1      1      1      1      !      II 

n 

1               5                   10      13      15                   20 

day 

Figure  5.  Sequence  oflearning  by  129  control  half  brains  to  take  a 
smooth  ball  and  reject  a  rough.  Four  trials  daily.  On  days  1 1  and  1 2. 
and  21  and  22,  the  figure  shows  mean  takes  out  of  24  unrewarded  tests 
with  each  ball  (Young,  1983). 


The  basic  action  of  the  system  is  that  an  arm  cautiously 
and  slowly  draws  in  an  unfamiliar  object  that  it  touches. 
If  this  proves  to  provide  food,  the  taste  signals  from  the 
lips  activate  cells  of  the  lateral  inferior  frontal,  which  in- 
crease the  tendency  to  take  and  perhaps  operate  as  a  com- 
petitive learning  matrix  (like  the  dentate  gyrus).  The  pat- 
tern of  input  and  taste  signals  is  then  passed  on  to  the 
median  inferior  frontal  where  that  proportion  of  cells  that 
receive  this  pattern  of  signals  of  touch  and  taste  is  acti- 
vated. This  can  be  considered  a  re-coding  of  the  input 
pattern  on  to  a  more  sparse  set  of  cells. 

The  axons  of  these  cells  then  proceed  through  the  inter- 
weaving bundles  to  the  subfrontal,  where  they  make  con- 
nection with  a  still  smaller  set  of  large  cells  with  complex 
dendritic  fields,  having  also  an  input  of  fibers  indicating 
pain.  If  trauma  occurs  and  these  pain  fibers  are  also  ac- 
tivated, then  the  large  cells  of  the  subfrontal  operate  the 
rejection  neurons  of  the  posterior  buccal  lobe.  The  syn- 
apses activated  by  this  particular  pattern  of  input  become 
consolidated,  presumably  by  the  action  of  the  large  num- 
ber of  amacrine  cells  whose  short  axons  end  among  the 
dendrites  of  the  larger  cells  of  the  subfrontal  lobe  (Fig.  6). 

The  basic  operation  of  the  system  is  thus  to  take  objects 
touched  unless  signals  of  pain  arrive.  Signals  of  taste  set 
up  a  greater  tendency  to  take  by  competitive  learning  in 
the  lateral  inferior  frontal  lobe.  Signals  of  pain  set  up  a 
tendency  to  reject  that  pattern  of  touch  by  modification 
of  synapses  in  the  subfrontal  lobe.  As  good  evidence  of 
this  it  was  found  that,  after  lesions  destroyed  all  the  small 


amacnne  cells,  an  octopus  tailed  to  learn  not  to  take  ob- 
jects from  which  shocks  were  received  (see  this  paper  and 
Wells.  1978). 

The  Vertical  Lobe  System  and  Touch  Learning 

The  inferior  frontal  system  contains  the  major  tactile 
memory,  but  the  vertical  lobe  also  contributes.  Experi- 
ments show  that  removal  of  the  vertical  lobe  impairs  the 
tactile  memory,  but  removal  of  the  median  inferior  frontal 
has  no  effect  on  visual  learning. 

The  tactile  signals  enter  the  vertical  lobe  circuit  through 
fibers  from  the  lateral  inferior  frontal  that  enter  the  outer 
plexus  of  the  lateral  superior  frontal  lobe  (Fig.  3).  The 
vertical  lobe  system  contains  four  lobes  precisely  similar 
to  those  we  have  described  in  the  inferior  frontal.  The 
lateral  superior  frontal  sends  fibers  to  the  subvertical  lobe 
and  from  there  fibers  pass  down  the  cerebral  tract  to  the 
posterior  buccal  lobe  (Fig.  6).  This  circuit  through  the 
lateral  superior  frontal  is  thus  in  a  position  to  increase 
still  further  the  tendency  to  take  objects  that  have  been 
associated  with  taste  reward. 

The  signals  for  touch  are  then  passed  on  from  the  lateral 
to  the  median  superior  frontal.  Here  the  bundles  are  again 
interwoven,  exactly  as  in  the  median  inferior  frontal.  The 
1.8  X  106  cells  thus  receive  varied  combinations  of  signals 
of  touch  and  taste,  and  these  are  passed  on  again  through 


medinf  f- 


certr 


Figure  6.     Diagram  of  connections  in  the  inferior  frontal  system  of 
an  octopus  (Young,  197  1 ). 


204 


J.  Z.  YOUNG 


Figure  7.  Transverse  section  of  the  inferior  frontal  system  of  Octopus 
vii/garis  stained  with  Cajal's  silver  method.  Abbreviations  as  Figure  4. 
cr.  tr..  cerebral  tract  (from  subvertical  lobe):  p.  tract  of  probably  pain 
fibers  from  hind  end  of  bodv. 


a  complex  plexus  to  the  vertical  lobe.  Here  there  are  rel- 
atively few  large  cells  (65,000),  with  complex  dendrites, 
exactly  like  those  of  the  subfrontal,  and  no  less  than  25 
million  amacrine  cells. 

The  large  cells  send  their  axons  down  to  the  subvertical 
lobe  and  so  to  the  posterior  buccal,  but  also  back  to  the 
lateral  superior  frontal  (Fig.  3).  This  circuit  evidently  plays 
some  part  in  re-enforcing  the  conjunctions,  possibly  by 
maintaining  particular  patterns  by  re-excitation. 

The  Distributed  Tactile  Learning  System 

The  system  for  touch  learning  thus  includes  no  less 
than  eight  distinct  lobes  with  matrix  structure  (Fig.  3). 
The  relative  parts  played  by  the  various  lobes  was  studied 
over  a  number  of  years  in  a  large  number  of  animals  with 
divided  brains.  Lesions  were  made  on  one  side,  and  the 
other  was  left  as  a  control.  In  many  of  the  experiments, 
discrimination  was  between  completely  smooth  balls  (0 
rings)  and  those  with  13  incisions.  The  sequence  of  train- 
ing for  129  normal  sides  is  shown  in  Figure  5. 

A  useful  measure  of  the  extent  and  reliability  of  dis- 
crimination is  to  give  a  series  of  24  extinction  tests  with 
balls  of  differing  roughness,  shown  at  short  intervals  (1- 
3  min)  without  any  reward.  Such  tests  are  arduous  to 
give,  but  they  show  that  habituation  proceeds  more  slowly 
in  proportion  to  similarity  of  each  ball  to  the  one  for 
which  reward  was  previously  given  (Fig.  8).  The  capacity 
for  discrimination  was  also  tested  by  using  more  nearly 
similar  balls,  with  4  and  7  rings.  With  long  training,  oc- 


topuses could  probably  make  some  discrimination  even 
between  a  difference  of  one  ring. 

By  such  tests  we  can  compare  the  discrimination  by 
animals  after  various  lesions.  Without  the  median  inferior 
frontal  there  is  still  discrimination,  but  it  is  much  less 
accurate  than  in  control  animals  (Fig.  9).  Removal  of  the 
vertical  lobe  also  reduces  accuracy,  although  to  a  lesser 
extent.  Clearly  each  of  the  lobes  through  which  the  in- 
formation passes  adds  something  to  the  effectiveness  of 
the  representations  that  are  formed,  as  would  be  expected 
from  a  system  of  matrices. 

Animals  without  vertical  lobes  show  errors  largely  when 
they  take  the  negative  ball,  showing  again  that  this  lobe 
serves  to  increase  the  effectiveness  of  shocks.  In  normal 
octopuses,  learning  is  possible  even  if  rewards  are  delayed 
for  up  to  30  s  after  the  ball  has  been  removed.  In  animals 
without  vertical  lobes,  such  delay  is  no  longer  possible 
(Wells  and  Young,  1968).  The  re-excitation  within  the 
vertical  lobe  system  serves  to  maintain  the  necessary  ex- 
citability for  a  Hebb  type  of  learning. 

We  have  used  this  technique  to  make  a  large  number 
of  experiments,  leaving  one  side  as  a  control.  With  this 
technique  it  is  possible  to  remove  the  subfrontal  lobe, 
which  cannot  be  approached  laterally.  The  effect  is  to 
produce  a  complete  inability  to  learn  on  that  side:  there 
is  a  strong  and  irreversible  preference  for  the  rougher  balls 
(Young,  1983).  The  lobe  evidently  has  some  specific  effect 
on  the  coding  and  discrimination  process. 

After  cutting  the  cerebro-brachial  tract  (Fig.  6),  the 
whole  influence  of  the  inferior  frontal  and  vertical  systems 
is  removed.  Nevertheless,  there  is  still  a  slight  capacity  for 
learned  discrimination  (Fig.  10),  which  must  lie  in  the 
suboesophageal  ganglia,  or  in  the  arms  themselves.  This 
residual  learning  is  difficult  to  study.  Animals  with  these 


CONTROL  HALF-BRAINS 


0       4       6       9       13 
number  of  rings 

Figure  8.  Tests  after  training  in  three  different  directions.  Means 
and  standard  errors  of  ratios  of  takes  of  each  ball  to  total  takes,  with  24 
trials  with  each  ball.  The  bars  show  standard  errors. 


COMPUTATION  IN  CEPHALOPOD  LEARNING  SYSTEM 


205 


~\NV 


"0469       13  0469       13  0469      13 

number  nf  rings 

Figure  9.  Tests  after  training.  Comparison  of  results  (a):  1 1  animals  after  cutting  the  tract  between 
superior  frontal  and  vertical  lobes  on  one  side  (NSFV)  and  removing  the  vertical  lobe  (NV)  on  the  other 
side;  36  controls  for  comparison  (b):  36  animals  after  removing  the  median  interior  frontal  (NMIF);  controls 
and  NV  added  for  comparison.  In  a  &  b,  all  training  was  with  smooth  balls  positive  and  13  rings  negative. 
In  (c)  balls  with  six  rings  were  positive  and  smooth  balls  negative.  The  figure  shows  the  results  for  nine 
animals  with  the  median  inferior  frontal  removed  on  one  side  three  animals  with  no  vertical  lobe  on  one 
side  added  for  comparison.  Note  that  here  (and  Fig.  8)  the  balls  with  9  and  13  rings  were  seldom  taken, 
although  they  had  not  been  associated  with  shock. 


large  operations  do  not  feed  well  and  tend  to  hold  pieces 
of  food  and  other  objects  close  to  the  mouth.  Nevertheless, 
the  differences  in  numbers  of  takes  of  the  balls  during 
tests  are  significant  and  show  that  some  learning  has  oc- 
curred. 

Some  measure  of  the  accuracy  of  the  memory  after  the 
various  lesions  is  given  by  the  difference  between  the  takes 
of  the  rough  and  smooth  balls  in  the  tests  (Table  I).  Using 
the  difference  in  controls  as  a  standard  (100%),  we  can 
judge  that  animals  without  vertical  lobes  are  rather  less 
than  half  as  efficient,  without  the  median  inferior  frontal 
are  one  third  as  efficient,  and  that  the  suboesophageal 
contribution  is  about  one  sixth.  Damage  to  the  subfrontal 
produces  a  perverse  effect. 

The  Visual  Memory  System 

The  vertical  lobe  system,  which  plays  a  part  in  tactile 
learning,  forms,  with  the  optic  lobes,  the  main  and  only 


control 

control        trained  13-t-_, 
trained  0-*-  n~L6 

n=36 


I   20 


NCB.13+ 


component  of  the  visual  learning  system.  The  existence 
of  this  double  capacity  is  a  striking  demonstration  of  the 
power  of  such  a  distributed  matrix  system  to  store  a  variety 
of  inputs.  It  will  be  very  interesting  to  investigate  whether 
individual  cells  of  the  vertical  lobe  system  play  a  part  in 
both  systems.  In  a  study  of  combined  visual  and  tactile 
training,  no  mutual  interactions  between  the  two  modal- 
ities was  seen  (Allen  ct  at.,  1986). 

The  study  of  visual  memory  is  made  difficult  by  the 
complexity  of  the  connections  in  the  optic  lobes,  which 
are  not  fully  understood.  Cells  with  large  tangential  den- 
drites  in  the  plexiform  layer  probably  act  as  feature  de- 
tectors (Fig.  1 1 ).  Their  axons  form  columns  proceeding 
to  the  center  of  the  lobe,  where  they  interact  in  an  inter- 
weaving matrix  of  cells  and  fibers.  Second  or  third  order 
visual  neurons  then  send  axons  to  the  central  nervous 


Table  I 

Mean  takes  in  final  tests  after  various  lesions 


04        6       9      13 
number  of  rings 

Figure  10.     Tests  after  training.  Comparison  of  control  sides  with 
those  with  the  cerebrobrachial  tracts  cut  (NCB). 


Smooth 

Rough 

Percent 

ball 

ball 

accuracy 

Lobe  removed 

(positive) 

(negative) 

Difference 

remaining 

None  (controls) 

20.22 

2.44 

17.78 

100 

Vertical 

17.68 

10.55 

7.13 

40 

Median  inferior 

frontal 

16.32 

10.69 

5.63 

32 

Cerebro-brachial 

tract 

13.78 

10.94 

2.84 

16 

Subfrontal 

7.89 

8.42 

-0.53 

— 

The  last  column  indicates  the  capacity  for  learned  discrimination  that 
remains  after  the  lesions,  estimated  as  a  percentage  of  the  differences  in 
the  controls. 


206 


J.  Z.  YOUNG 


vert 


med  sup  f r 
I  at  sup  fr 


buc 


op. 


ret 


V 


Figure  11.  Diagram  of  the  connections  of  the  visual  and  tactile 
learning  system  of  an  octopus.  1,  retina;  2,  second  order  visual  cells 
(feature  detectors):  3,  centrifugal  cells;  4,  amacrine  cells;  5,  tangential 
cells;  6,  optic-peduncle;  7.  optic-magnocellular;  8,  optic-anterior  basal; 
9.  optic-median  basal;  10.  optic-lateral  superior  frontal;  1 1,  optic-median 
superior  frontal;  12.  taste  fibers-lateral  inferior  frontal;  13,  taste  fibers 
median  inferior  frontal;  14,  lateral  inferior  frontal-lateral  superior  frontal; 
15.  lateral  inferior  frontal-median  superior  frontal;  16,  median  superior 
frontal-vertical;  17,  vertical-lateral  superior  frontal;  18,  lateral  superior 
frontal-subvertical;  19,  pain  fibers-vertical:  20,  pain  fibers-subfrontal;  2 1 , 
vertical-subvertical:  22,  subvertical-optic;  23,  subvertical-precommissural: 
24,  precommissural-palliovisceral;  25,  precommissural-magnocellular; 
26,  subvertical-posterior  buccal;  27,  chemo-tactile  libers-lateral  inferior 
frontal;  28,  chemo-tactile  fibers-median  inferior  frontal;  29,  median  in- 
ferior frontal-subfrontal;  30.  subfrontal-postenor  buccal;  31,  lateral  in- 
ferior frontal-posterior  buccal;  32.  motor  fibers  from  posterior  buccal  to 
arms. 


system  (Fig.  1 1).  Some  pass  to  the  magnocellular  lobe, 
and  this  is  probably  a  pathway  for  rapid  escape  reactions. 
Other  fibers  pass  to  the  peduncle  and  basal  lobes,  which 
together  regulate  movement,  including  attack.  A  third 
pathway  leads  to  the  superior  frontal,  and  so  to  the  vertical 
lobe,  and  is  responsible  for  learned  behavior. 

The  system  is  organized  exactly  as  we  have  seen  for 
tactile  learning.  In  the  lateral  superior  frontal,  the  visual 
fibers  interact  with  those  of  taste,  and  this  is  a  pathway 
that  promotes  attack.  After  removing  this  lobe  from  one 
side,  an  octopus  will  no  longer  attack  when  that  eye  has 
been  used  to  see  a  crab,  for  instance,  at  a  distance  (Boycott 
and  Young,  1955).  The  median  superior  frontal  and  ver- 
tical lobes  provide  a  system  that  prevents  visual  attack 
when  trauma  occurs.  After  removal  of  these  lobes  or  an 


interruption  of  the  circuit,  an  octopus  will  continue  to 
make  attacks,  even  at  crabs,  in  spite  of  receiving  shocks, 
unless  these  shocks  are  given  at  intervals  of  five  minutes 
or  less.  "The  setting-up  of  a  memory  representing  asso- 
ciation of  a  given  situation  with  a  shock  is  therefore  a 
property  of  the  optic  and  basal  lobes  but  persistence  of 
the  representation  depends  upon  the  presence  of  the  ver- 
tical lobe"  (Boycott  and  Young,  1955). 

Many  other  experiments  have  confirmed  that  learning 
of  visual  discrimination  is  impaired  by  lesions  of  the  ver- 
tical lobe  system  (Young,  1961.  1965).  If  part  of  the  ver- 
tical lobe  is  removed,  the  accuracy  of  the  memory  is  pro- 
portionately reduced.  This  "graceful  degradation"  is  a 
property  to  be  expected  in  such  a  distributed  system.  In- 
cidentally, Boycott  and  I  were  able  to  show  that  the  same 
is  true  of  the  optic  lobes.  Memories  are  retained  after  re- 
moval of  at  least  50%  of  the  lobe  or  after  making  lesions 
in  several  places  with  a  cataract  knife. 

Discussion 

The  two  memory  systems  of  an  octopus  thus  work  on 
precisely  similar  principles.  The  input  signals  are  passed 
through  a  series  of  matrices  of  intersecting  axes  allowing 
for  particular  groupings  of  signals  to  interact  and  to  be 
directed  to  the  pathways  for  attack  or  retreat.  The  systems 
are  tuned  to  produce  exploratory  investigation  of  novel 
situations.  If  the  results  are  favorable,  the  particular  set 
of  connections  in  the  lateral  frontal  lobes  are  re-enforced 
by  signals  of  taste,  and  this  set  later  produces  more  rapid 
attacks  or  takes  by  the  arms.  The  inputs  are  given  further 
opportunity  for  interaction  in  the  matrices  of  the  median 
frontal  lobes.  In  the  vertical  and  subfrontal  lobes,  partic- 
ular sets  are  then  concentrated  into  rather  few  large  cells. 
The  recurrent  output  from  these  to  the  lateral  superior 
frontal  lobe  presumably  re-enforces  the  tendency  to  pos- 
itive action,  unless  pain  occurs.  In  that  case,  the  other 
outputs  from  these  large  cells  of  the  vertical  or  subfrontal 
prevent  further  investigation  of  that  configuration  of  in- 
puts. The  numerous  amacrine  cells  in  these  lobes  are  ev- 
idently concerned  with  establishing  the  conjunction  be- 
tween particular  sets  of  input  signals  and  the  pathways  of 
retreat. 

The  organization  of  these  lobes,  and  the  effects  of  re- 
moving them,  suggests  that  learning  in  these  animals  is 
not  localized  to  one  or  two  "hidden  layers"  or  to  a  few 
essential  "grandmother  cells,"  but  is  distributed  with  high 
redundancy  in  a  series  of  matrices  networks,  with  recur- 
rent circuitry,  up  to  a  late  stage  where  funneling  to  a  few 
cells  occurs. 

We  can  gain  some  insight  into  how  this  process  has 
evolved  by  considering  the  differences  between  octopods 
and  decapods.  Cuttlefishes  and  squids  have  a  system  of 
matrices  for  visual  learning  similar  to  that  of  octopods 


COMPUTATION  IN  CEPHALOPOD  LEARNING  SYSTEM 

vert  B 


207 


Figure  12.  Sagittal  section  of  the  brain  of  Sepia.  Note  that  there  is 
no  median  interior  frontal  or  subfrontal.  The  superior  frontal  has  a  matrix 
structure  like  that  of  Octopus.  The  vertical  lobe  has  a  rather  different 
structure.  Cajal  silver  stain,  b.a.,  anterior  basal;  h.  med.,  median  basal; 
fr.  ]..  interior  frontal;  fr.  s.,  superior  frontal;  prec.,  precommissural;  subv., 
subvertical;  v..  vertical. 


(Fig.  12).  In  an  early  experiment  it  was  shown  that  inter- 
ruption of  the  vertical  lobe  circuit  damages  the  visual 
memory  system  of  Sepia  (Young,  1938;  Sanders  and 
Young.  1940).  This  was  the  first  suggestion  that  the  cir- 
culation of  impulses  around  a  circuit  provides  a  basis  for 
memory  (Fig.  13).  There  has  been  little  further  progress 
because  the  experiments  are  more  difficult  than  in  octo- 
pods.  In  decapods,  the  inferior  frontal  system  is  much 
simpler  than  in  octopods:  there  is  no  median  inferior 
frontal  or  subfrontal  lobe.  These  animals  detect  the  prey 
visually  and  often  seize  by  ejection  of  the  tentacles.  It 
seems  likely  that  they  have,  at  best,  only  a  small  capacity 
for  learned  tactile  discrimination;  the  operations  of  ma- 
nipulating and  eating  the  prey  are  complex,  but  are  prob- 
ably largely  reflex.  Nevertheless,  there  must  be  a  mecha- 
nism for  release  of  any  object  that  gives  pain  when  it  is 
held.  Probably  all  reflex  systems  have  some  method  of 
inhibition,  especially  if  they  involve  muscles  acting  recip- 
rocally, such  as  flexors  and  extensors  in  mammals,  where 
the  inhibition  is  produced  by  Golgi  type  II  cells  in  the 
spinal  cord.  In  cephalopods,  reciprocal  inhibition  is  prob- 
ably produced  by  the  smaller  amacrine  neurons  that  are 
common  among  the  larger  motorneurons  of  the  superior 
buccal  and  suboesophageal  centers  (Fig.  14).  These  mi- 


Figure  13.  An  early  suggestion  of  re-excitation  as  the  basis  of  memory. 
Diagram  of  Scpiu  to  show  how  circulation  between  the  lateral  superior 
frontal  (lat.  sup.  fr.)  and  vertical  (vert.)  might  facilitate  the  firing  of  a 
motorneuron  (M)  by  conjunctive  excitation  from  the  optic  lobe  (O)  and 
taste  fibers  (V)( Young. 


croneurons  have  processes  restricted  to  a  limited  field, 
where  they  may  serve  to  repress  activity  in  the  larger  cells. 
In  this  context,  it  is  especially  interesting  that  we  found 
some  simple  capacity  for  tactile  memory  in  the  suboe- 
sophageal lobes. 

It  is  suggested  that  the  amacrine  cells  of  the  subfrontal 
and  vertical  lobes  of  octopods  have  evolved  from  inhib- 
itors of  the  reciprocal  feeding  reflexes.  The  inferior  frontal 
and  vertical  lobe  systems  are  backward  extensions  of  the 
superior  buccal  lobes  (see  Fig.  4).  The  matrices  that  are 
responsible  for  learning  have  evolved  by  the  modification 
of  these  simpler  reflex  centers.  The  incoming  afferent  fibers 
have  become  marshalled  into  rows  crossing  the  axons  of 
cells  of  the  lobe,  allowing  the  formation  of  conjunctive 
response  to  the  incoming  patterns  of  stimuli.  The  ama- 
crine cells  became  collected  together  in  distinct  lobes, 
serving  to  prolong  the  effects,  perhaps  especially  of  inhib- 


Figure  14.     Drawing  ofa  single  large  cell  from  the  pedal  lobe  of  Loligo. 
accompanied  by  very  small  cells  with  branches  in  the  neighborhood. 


208 


J.  Z.  YOUNG 


itory  inputs.  The  details  are  far  from  clear,  but  this  pro- 
vides a  possible  scenario  for  the  evolution  of  memory 
mechanisms,  at  least  among  cephalopods. 

Acknowledgments 

The  work  reported  here  has  been  helped  by  many  col- 
leagues. I  am  especially  grateful  to  Brian  Boycott,  Martin 
Wells,  Marion  Nixon,  and  Pamela  Stephens.  The  Stazione 
Zoologica  at  Naples  provided  excellent  conditions  for  ex- 
periment over  many  years.  Recently,  work  has  been 
helped  by  grants  from  The  Wellcome  Trust  and  University 
College,  London.  I  am  grateful  to  Professor  L.  Weiskrantz, 
F.R.S.,  for  accommodation  in  the  Psychology  Department 
at  Oxford  and  to  my  wife  Raye  for  her  secretarial  help 
and  typing.  Dr.  M.  J.  Wells  kindly  commented  on  an 
early  draft  of  the  paper. 

Literature  Cited 

Allen,  A.,  J.  Michels,  and  J.  Z.  Young.  1986.  Possible  interactions 
between  visual  and  tactile  memories  in  octopus.  Mar.  Behav.  Physiol. 
12:81-97. 

Boycott,  B.  B.,  and  J.  Z.  Young.  1955.  A  memory  system  in  Octopus 
vulgaris  Lamark.  Prof.  R  Soc.  B  143:  449-480. 


Budelmann,  B.-l .,  and  J.  Z.  Young.  1985.  Central  nervous  pathways 
for  the  arms  and  mantle  of  Octopus.  Phil.  Trans.  R  Soc.  B  310: 
109-122. 

Hopfield.J.  J.  1982.  Neural  networks  and  physical  systems  with  emer- 
gent collective  computational  abilities.  Proc  Natl.  Acad.  Sci.  USA 
79:  2554-2558. 

Rolls,  E.  T.  1990.  The  representation  and  storage  of  information  in 
neural  networks  in  the  primate  cerebral  cortex  and  hippocampus.  In 
The  Computing  Neuron.  R.  Durbin.  C.  Miall,  and  G.  Mitchison,  eds. 
Addison-Wesley,  Wokingham. 

Sanders.  F.  K.,  and  J.  Z.  Young.  1940.  Learning  and  other  functions 
of  the  higher  nervous  centres  of  Sepia.  J.  Neurophysiol.  3:  501-526. 

Wells,  M.  J.  1978.  Octopus.  In  Physiology  and  Behaviour  of  an  Ad- 
vanced Invertebrate.  Chapman  and  Hall.  London. 

Wells,  M.  J.,  and  J.  Z.  Young.  1968.  Learning  with  delayed  rewards 
in  Octopus.  Z  I'ergl.  Physiol.  61:  103-128. 

Young,  J.  Z.  1938.  The  evolution  of  the  nervous  system  and  of  the 
relationship  of  organism  and  environment.  Pp.  1 79-204  in  Evolution. 
essays  presented  to  E.  S.  Goodrich,  G.  R.  de  Beer.  ed.  Clarendon 
Press,  Oxford. 

Young,  J.  Z.  1961.  Learning  and  discrimination  in  the  octopus.  Biol 
Rev  36:  32-96. 

Young,  J.  Z.  1965.  The  organization  of  a  memory  system.  The  Crooman 
Lecture.  Proc.  R  Soc.  B  163:  285-320. 

Young,  J.  Z.  1971.  The  Anatomy  of  the  Nervous  System  of  Octopus 
vulgaris.  Clarendon  Press.  Oxford. 

Young,  J.  Z.  1983.  The  distributed  tactile  memory  system  of  Octopus. 
Proc.  R  Soc.  B  218:  135-176. 


Reference:  Biol  Bull  180:  209-220.  (April.  1991) 


Development  of  Giant  Motor  Axons  and 

Neural  Control  of  Escape  Responses 

in  Squid  Embryos  and  Hatchlings 

W.  F.  GILLY,  BRUCE  HOPKINS,  AND  G.  O.  MACKIE* 

Hopkins  Marine  Station,  Department  of  Biological  Sciences, 
Slanf'onl  University,  Pacific  Grove,  California  93950 


Abstract.  Anatomical  development  of  the  third-order 
giant  axons  was  studied  in  conjunction  with  ontogeny  of 
the  escape  response  and  the  underlying  neural  control. 
Stimulated  escape  jetting  appears  at  stage  26  (Segawa  el 
a/..  1988):  such  responses  are  driven  solely  by  a  small 
axon  motor  system.  Giant  axons  become  morphologically 
identifiable  in  the  more  posterior  stellar  nerves  that  effect 
jetting  by  stage  28,  and  electrical  activity  in  the  stellate 
ganglia  associated  with  the  giant  axons  is  first  recordable 
at  this  time.  Maturation  of  the  giant  axons  is  accompanied 
by  a  marked  improvement  in  temporal  aspects  of  escape 
behavior  up  to  the  time  of  hatching.  In  embryonic  and 
hatchling  Loligo.  all  escape  responses,  regardless  of  the 
mode  of  stimulation,  are  fast-start  responses  with  latencies 
less  than  the  minimum  value  displayed  by  adults  (50  ms). 
Giant  axon  activity  recorded  in  the  stellate  ganglion  always 
precedes  small  axon  motor  activity;  this  is  not  true  for 
adults  which  display  two  distinct  modes  of  giant  axon 
use.  Both  giant  and  non-giant  motor  systems  are  thus 
functional  in  embryonic  and  hatchling  squid,  and  both 
contribute  to  escape  jetting.  However,  these  animals  do 
not  yet  display  the  concerted  interplay  of  the  two  motor 
systems  characteristic  of  adults. 

Introduction 

Lolliginid  squid  possess  giant  neurons  with  very  large 
axons  that  are  important  components  of  the  motor  path- 
ways mediating  jet-propelled  escape  responses  (Young, 
1938).  Anatomical  details  of  the  giant  fiber  pathway  were 
beautifully  described  at  the  light  microscope  level  over  50 
years  ago  by  Young  ( 1939).  In  brief,  two  bilaterally  sym- 


Received  15  August  1990;  accepted  6  November  1990. 
*  Permanent  Address:  Department  of  Biology,  University  of  Victoria, 
Victoria.  B.C.,  Canada. 


metrical  first  order  interneurons  lie  in  the  magnocellular 
lobe  of  the  brain  and  receive  massive  sensory  inputs  from 
many  sources,  including  the  statocysts,  optic  lobes,  and 
mechanoreceptors  in  the  tentacles.  These  cells  are  unusual 
in  that  their  short  axons  are  fused  via  a  cytoplasmic  bridge. 
Each  first-order  giant  axon  contacts  seven  second-order 
giant  cells  in  the  palliovisceral  lobe.  The  largest  of  these 
cells  are  interneurons,  and  each  projects  a  giant  axon  via 
the  pallial  nerve  to  the  ipsilateral  stellate  ganglion  where 
it  contacts  the  third-order  giant  motor  axons  of  the  stellar 
nerves.  The  six  other  second-order  giant  cells  are  moto- 
neurons  that  innervate  the  musculature  associated  with 
head  retraction,  siphon  aiming,  and  ink  ejection. 

This  basic  plan  is  straightforward,  but  oversimplified. 
Each  of  the  above  giant  cells  receives  numerous  other 
synaptic  inputs,  about  which  little  is  known  (Boyle,  1986). 
The  first-order  giants  also  receive  major  inputs  from 
higher-order  centers  such  as  the  cerebral  ganglia.  Inte- 
grated outputs  from  these  regions  must  influence  activity 
of  the  first-order  giants,  which  are  likely  to  be  an  important 
decision-making  element,  given  the  commanding  ana- 
tomical position  they  hold.  Complexity  in  the  giant  fiber 
pathway  and  the  biological  necessity  to  strictly  control 
giant  fiber  excitation  was  clearly  recognized  by  Young 
(1939),  who  pointed  out  that  lack  of  such  control  ".  .  . 
would  lead  to  behavior  by  the  squid  even  more  'nervous' 
than  that  for  which  the  animals  have  a  reputation." 

Despite  the  wealth  of  anatomical  data,  physiological 
studies  of  squid  escape  behavior  have  lagged  far  behind 
work  on  many  other  preparations  (Eaton,  1984;  Mackie, 
1990).  The  pioneering  work  of  Young  [1938;  Prosserand 
Young  (1937)],  in  which  reflex  activation  of  escape  jetting 
was  inferred  from  studies  of  nerve-mantle  preparations, 
was  followed  up  only  much  later  by  Wilson  (1960),  who 
studied  control  of  mantle  contractions  by  a  small  axon 


209 


210 


W.  F.  GILLY  ET  AL 


system  as  well  as  by  the  giant  axons.  Since  that  time  it 
has  become  widely  accepted  that  ( 1 )  high-pressure  escape 
jetting  is  mediated  solely  by  the  giant  axon  pathway  es- 
sentially as  a  reflex  and  that  (2)  low-pressure  respiratory 
'jetting'  is  the  primary,  if  not  only,  function  of  the  small 
axon  (non-giant)  motor  system. 

Reinvestigation  of  these  ideas  has  revealed  a  more 
complex  picture  (Otis  and  Gilly.  1990).  Recordings  of 
stellar  nerve  activity  during  escape  responses  //;  vivo  show 
that  strong  escape  jets  can  be  driven  by  the  small  axon 
system  acting  independently  or  in  concert  with  the  giant 
axons.  Moreover,  the  giant  axon  pathway  can  be  used  in 
two  distinct  modes.  One  produces  a  short  latency  startle 
response,  whereas  the  second  leads  to  a  complex  delayed 
escape  response.  In  the  latter  case,  critically  timed  exci- 
tation of  the  giants  provides  a  potent,  but  secondary  boost 
to  the  jet. 

The  present  study  investigates  the  neural  control  of  es- 
cape jetting  in  embryonic  and  hatchling  squid.  All  three 
neural  elements  of  the  giant  fiber  pathway  are  highly  de- 
veloped at  the  time  of  hatching  (Young,  1939),  and  escape 
jetting  capabilities  are  respectable  (Packard,  1969).  Em- 
bryonic development  of  the  first-order  giant  neurons  has 
been  studied  in  detail  (Martin.  1965,  1969,  1977),  but 
much  less  is  known  about  the  second-  and  third-order 
giants  (Marthy,  1987).  Behavioral  and  neurophysiological 
studies  concerning  development  of  the  giant  fiber  pathway 
are  completely  lacking. 

This  paper  describes  the  ontogeny  of  the  escape  re- 
sponse in  relation  to  development  of  the  giant  fiber  system, 
with  emphasis  on  the  role  of  the  third-order  motor  axons. 
Several  questions  are  addressed,  answers  to  which  provide 
the  framework  for  future  studies.  When  do  the  giant  motor 
axons  develop  morphologically,  and  when  do  they  begin 
to  mediate  escape  responses'?  What  functional  conse- 
quences are  manifested  in  parallel  with  growth  of  the  giant 
axons?  How  does  the  pattern  of  giant  axons  use  compare 
to  that  in  the  adult  animal?  Finally,  when  does  controlled 
interplay  between  the  giant  and  non-giant  motor  systems 
develop? 


Materials  and  Methods 


Animals 


Loligo  opalescens  was  collected  from  Monterey  Bay 
and  maintained  in  holding  tanks  plumbed  with  flow- 
through  seawater  (~  13°C).  Spawning  occurred  in  these 
tanks,  and  clusters  of  fertilized  eggs  were  maintained  in 
small  mesh  enclosures  in  gently  flowing  seawater  until 
natural  hatching  occurred  ( ~30  days).  Staging  of  animals 
was  carried  out  following  criteria  of  Segawa  et  ai  ( 1988) 
with  only  minor  modifications  (see  Results  and  Table  I). 

Sepioteuthis  lessoniana  was  supplied  by  the  Marine 
Biomedical  Institute,  University  of  Texas  Medical  Branch, 
Galveston,  where  the  animals  had  been  reared.  Animals 


ranging  from  5  to  2 1  days  post-hatching  were  shipped 
overnight  and  studied  the  following  day. 

Behavioral  experiments 

LoHgo  from  stage  24  through  post-hatching  was  used 
for  studying  development  of  escape-jetting  behavior.  To 
obtain  animals  for  each  experiment,  a  "finger"  of  eggs 
was  disrupted,  the  eggs  were  dispersed  in  seawater,  and 
the  chorionic  membranes  were  ruptured  to  release  the 
embryos.  Individual  embryos  were  chosen  from  this  pool 
and  staged  under  a  stereomicroscope. 

After  staging,  an  animal  was  placed  in  a  35  mm  culture 
dish  lined  with  Sylgard  (Dow-Corning,  Midland,  Michi- 
gan) and  filled  with  seawater  at  room  temperature  ( 16- 
20°C).  The  animal  was  lightly  restrained  (ventral  surface 
up)  with  a  wire  yoke,  which  was  formed  to  fit  over  the 
base  of  the  arms  and  inserted  into  the  Sylgard.  Electrical 
stimuli  (20-60  V.  6  ms  duration)  were  delivered  via  a 
seawater-filled  micropipette  positioned  near  the  ventral 
midline  in  the  area  of  the  brachial  ganglion  (see  Fig.  2A). 

Behavioral  responses  were  recorded  on  conventional 
videotape.  The  stimulating  pulse  also  triggered  a  small 
light  source  beneath  the  experimental  chamber,  which 
served  as  a  timing  marker.  Although  the  exact  frame  when 
the  stimulus  occurred  is  identifiable  with  this  method, 
stimuli  were  not  synchronized  with  the  video  framing  and 
uncertainty  therefore  exists  about  precisely  when  in  the 
frame  the  pulse  occurred  (each  video  frame  spans  33  ms). 
Data  were  later  sampled  in  blocks  of  16  real-time  frames 
with  an  image-analysis  system  (Megavision  1024  XM. 
Santa  Barbara.  California)  and  analyzed  on  a  frame-by- 
frame  basis  to  determine  the  time  course  of  the  change 
in  mantle  diameter  following  stimulation  (see  Fig.  2B). 

Electrophysiologiccd  experiments:  adult  squid 

Adult  specimens  of  Loligo  were  lightly  anesthetized  in 
0.4%  urethane  in  aerated  seawater  at  15°C.  The  mantle 
was  slit  ventrally  along  the  midline,  exposing  the  gills, 
and  the  squid  was  pinned  ventral  side  up  through  the 
outspread  mantle  flaps  to  the  bottom  of  a  Sylgard-filled 
glass  dish.  A  pin  through  the  region  of  the  mouth  pre- 
vented head  retraction.  The  left  pallial  nerve  was  cut 
proximal  to  the  stellate  ganglion,  thereby  immobilizing 
the  mantle  musculature  on  that  side.  On  the  right  side, 
the  stellate  ganglion  was  exposed  by  removing  the  thin 
layer  of  skin  that  covers  it.  The  larger,  more  posterior 
stellar  nerves  were  severed  close  to  their  emergence  from 
the  ganglion,  but  the  pallial  nerve  was  left  intact.  This 
paralyzed  the  right  side  of  the  animal  but  retained  synaptic 
transmission  at  the  giant  synapses  and  motor  outputs  in 
the  stellar  nerve  stumps.  Throughout  these  operations, 
chilled  seawater  was  kept  flowing  over  the  gills.  Upon 
completion  of  surgery,  the  urethane-seawater  was  ex- 


DEVELOPMENT  OF  SQUID  ESCAPE  RESPONSE 


211 


changed  for  O:-saturated  seawater  ( 12-15°C)  which  was 
thereafter  perfused  continuously  over  the  gills. 

Conventional  extracellular  recording  techniques  were 
used.  One  polyethylene  suction  electrode  was  attached  to 
the  pallial  nerve  for  en  passant  recording.  A  second  elec- 
trode was  used  to  record  from  a  stellar  nerve  by  sucking 
the  proximal  stump  into  the  lumen  of  the  electrode.  Volt- 
age was  measured  differentially  between  Ag:AgCl  wires, 
one  in  the  electrode's  lumen  and  the  other  wrapped 
around  the  tip.  Polarity  of  the  recordings  was  arranged 
so  that  the  initial  phase  of  activity  in  the  third-order  giant 
axon  was  positive-going.  AC-coupled  amplifiers  served  to 
amplify  and  low-pass  filter  (3-10  KHz)  the  signals,  which 
were  displayed  on  a  digital  oscilloscope  or  recorded  onto 
videotape  via  a  digital  audio  processor  (sampling  at  44 
KHz;  Unitrade,  Philadelphia,  Pennsylvania)  for  subse- 
quent analysis  using  a  laboratory  computer. 

Electrophysiological  experiments:  embryonic  and 
hatchling  squill 

Recording  techniques  were  adapted  from  those  used 
with  the  adult  animals.  Hatchlings  and  embryos  (removed 
from  their  chorions  and  staged  as  in  the  behavioral  ex- 
periments) were  pinned  out  with  fine  cactus  spines,  ventral 
side  up,  after  slitting  the  ventral  mantle  wall  to  allow  access 
to  the  stellate  ganglia.  No  nerves  were  cut,  and  recordings 
from  the  stellate  ganglion  were  made  with  a  suction  elec- 
trode applied  directly  over  the  ganglion  through  the  over- 
lying tissue.  Recordings  from  the  magnocellular  lobe  (site 
of  the  first-order  giant  neurons)  required  removal  of  the 
skin  and  cartilaginous  material  just  anterior  to  the  stato- 
cysts,  and  the  recording  electrode  was  applied  directly  to 
the  nervous  tissue  thereby  exposed.  It  was  also  necessary 
to  remove  some  of  the  mass  of  small  cells,  presumably 
undifferentiated  neuroblasts  (Martin,  1965),  which  overlie 
the  first-order  giant  cells  by  sucking  or  blowing  water  jets 
through  the  electrode  tip  before  attaching  it. 

For  all  neurophysiological  recordings,  electrical  stimuli 
were  delivered  through  a  fine  coaxial  metal  electrode,  and 
a  triggered  strobe  light  was  used  for  delivering  photic 
stimuli.  Shocks  of  1-3  ms.  20-80  V  were  most  effective 
in  stimulating  adults  and  0.1-0.4  ms,  10-50  V  in  hatch- 
lings  and  embryos.  All  experiments  were  carried  out  at 
12-15°C  in  the  case  ofLoligo  and  2 1  °C  with  Sepioteutliis. 

Electron  microscopy 

Staged  embryos  were  fixed  in  a  0.965  osmolar  solution 
of  1%  glutaraldehyde,  0.1%  tannic  acid.  0.2  M  sodium 
cacodylate  and  sucrose  (pH  7.4)  and  post-fixed  in  2%  os- 
mium tetroxide,  0.8%  potassium  ferrocyanide,  and  0.2  M 
sodium  cacodylate  (pH  6.8).  Fixed  material  was  dehy- 
drated in  graded  hexalene  glycol  and  embedded  in  Spurr's 
resin.  Animals  were  sectioned  both  perpendicular  and 
oblique  to  the  long  body  axis  to  obtain  transverse  sections 


of  the  posterior  stellar  nerves.  En  bloc  staining  was  carried 
out  in  saturated  uranyl  acetate  at  60°C  for  3  h.  Thin  sec- 
tions were  examined  in  a  Philips  30 1  transmission  electron 
microscope  operating  at  60  kV. 

Results 

Developmental  timetable  of  giant  motor  a.\ons  and 
jetting  behavior 

Loligo  opalescens  develops  embryologically  in  close 
correspondence  to  L.  forbesi,  and  staging  criteria  for  the 
latter  species  (Segawa  et  al.,  1988)  can  be  directly  applied. 
Stages  24  through  hatching  (30),  representing  approxi- 
mately half  of  the  total  developmental  period,  are  relevant 
to  the  present  study,  and  selected  characteristics  applicable 
to  our  staging  of  L.  opalescens  are  summarized  in  Table 
I.  Based  on  our  work  with  hundreds  of  embryos  in  this 
study,  ontogeny  of  jetting  behavior  can  be  related  to  these 
anatomical  stages  with  a  high  degree  of  confidence,  and 
this  information  is  included  in  Table  I  and  covered  in 
detail  below. 

Electron  microscopic  examination  of  conventionally 
fixed  and  embedded  material  reveals  that  axons  in  stellar 
nerves  which  can  be  labeled  'giant'  (i.e.,  distinctly  larger 
than  any  other  processes)  first  occur  at  stage  26,  but  only 
in  the  more  anterior  nerves  emanating  from  the  stellate 
ganglion  (Fig.  1A).  The  more  posterior  stellar  nerves  do 


Table  I 

Staging  characteristics  employed  and  development  of  escape  behavior 
in  embryonic  Loligo  opalescens.  Developmental  staging  follows 
the  criteria  described  by  Segawa  et  al.  (1988)  for  Loligo  forbesi. 
except  in  our  mirk  \\e  lumped  stages  27  and  27+  (into  27) 
and  28  and  2S+  (into  28) 

Stage  25:  1.  Spontaneous,  symmetrical  mantle  contractions  begin. 

Stage  26:  1.  Eyes  are  brilliant  red. 

2.  Ink  sac  is  visible. 

3.  Chromatophores  are  on  dorsal  side  of  head. 

4.  Electrical  and  mechanical  stimulation  of  escape  response 
is  possible. 

Stage  27:  1.  Ink  is  barely  visible  in  ink  sac. 

2.  Eyes  are  dark,  but  not  black. 

3.  Edge  of  primary  lid  covers  half  of  optic  vesicle. 

Stage  28:  1.  Eye  is  completely  covered  by  primary  lid. 

2.  Mid-gut  gland  is  visible. 

3.  Yellow  chromatophores  are  on  arms. 

4.  Head  retraction  occurs  during  escape  responses. 

Stage  29:  1.  Olfactory  organ  is  visible  as  thickened  disk. 
2.  Light  flashes  stimulate  escape  responses. 

Stage  29+:  1.  External  yolk  sac  is  equal  to  arm  length. 
Stage  29+  +  :  1.  Spontaneous  vigorous  jetting  occurs. 
Stage  30:  1.  Natural  hatching  occurs;  no  external  yolk  sac. 


212 


W.  F.  GILLY  ET  AL. 


C         Stage  28 


Stage  30 


Figure  1.  Anatomical  development  ot  the  giant  axons  in  stellar  nerves  of  embryonic  Loligo  opalescens. 
Each  panel  shows  a  cross-section  of  a  stellar  nerve  at  the  indicated  developmental  stage.  (A)  A  single  large 
axonal  process  is  first  identifiable  in  an  anterior  stellar  nerve  at  stage  26  (*),  although  other  processes  are 


DEVELOPMENT  OF  SQUID  ESCAPE  RESPONSE 


213 


not  display  a  singularly  large  axon  leading  to  the  mantle 
muscle  at  this  time  (Fig.  IB),  but  clearly  do  so  by  stage 
28  (Fig.  1C).  As  discussed  below,  stage  28  is  the  first  time 
at  which  giant  axon  activity  could  be  recorded  from  the 
stellate  ganglion.  Well-developed  giant  axons  exist  in  all 
stellar  nerves  at  the  time  of  hatching,  and  an  example  of 
a  hind-most  nerve  is  shown  in  Figure  ID. 

We  have  not  characterized  the  apparent  anterior  to 
posterior  wave  of  giant  axon  maturation  in  detail,  nor  do 
we  at  this  time  have  a  complete  picture  of  where,  when, 
and  how  the  axons  of  giant  fiber  lobe  motoneurons  ac- 
tually fuse  to  form  the  giant  axons  prior  to  stage  28.  Giant 
processes  can  be  identified  in  all  stellar  nerves  at  stage  27, 
but  (in  every  case  examined)  only  proximal  to  the  first 
branch  point  of  a  stellar  nerve  shortly  after  it  enters  the 
mantle  tissue. 

Development  of  escape  response:  behavioral  studies 

Escape  responses  in  embryonic  and  hatchling  squid 
were  stimulated  with  electrical  shocks,  strobe  light  flashes, 
or  mechanical  stimuli  and  videotaped  for  analysis.  An 
example  of  such  data  from  a  stage  29  embryo  is  shown 
in  Figure  2A.  Sequential  video  frames,  photographed  from 
the  display  monitor  of  the  image  analysis  system,  are 
numbered  —  3  through  4.  A  brief  electrical  stimulus  was 
applied  during  frame  0,  and  the  timing  is  identifiable  by 
a  light  flash  marker  (*).  Mantle  diameter  is  indicated  by 
arrowheads  in  each  panel,  and  Figure  2B  illustrates  the 
time  course  of  mantle  contraction  ( 1  frame  equals  33  ms). 
After  a  delay  of  one  frame,  mantle  diameter  decreases  to 
40%  of  its  original  (time  0)  value  over  the  subsequent  four 
frames. 

Peak  response,  delay,  and  time  to  peak  (as  indicated 
in  Fig.  2B)  were  measured  for  electrically  stimulated  jets 
in  3-4  animals  of  every  developmental  stage  from  25 
through  several  days  post-hatching.  Mean  data  are  sum- 
marized in  Figures  3A-B,  along  with  values  obtained  in 
similar  experiments  on  adult  animals  in  a  previous  study 
(Otis  and  Gilly,  1990). 

Escape  jetting  increases  in  strength  (Fig.  3A)  smoothly 
up  to  hatching  and  then  begins  to  slowly  decline.  Electrical 
stimuli  (•)  can  elicit  relatively  strong  responses  at  stages 
26-27,  a  time  when  there  is  no  anatomical  sign  of  giant 
axons  in  the  more  posterior  stellar  nerves  that  innervate 
the  mantle  area  in  which  diameter  was  monitored.  Pre- 
sumably these  responses  are  mediated  by  the  small  motor 
axon  system,  which  also  can  drive  strong  escape  jets  in 
adults.  There  is  no  dramatic  sign  of  increased  strength  in 


the  response  accompanying  the  appearance  of  functional 
posterior  giant  axons  by  stage  28.  Mechanical  stimulation 
(A;  taps  with  a  fine  probe)  also  leads  to  strong  escape  jets, 
and  light  flashes  (O)  become  effective  at  stage  29. 

A  functional  correlate  of  the  development  of  the  giant 
axons  in  the  posterior  stellar  nerves  is  suggested  in  Figure 
3B.  Both  time-to-peak  response  (•)  and  delay  (•)  decrease 
dramatically  between  stage  29  and  hatching.  As  described 
below,  giant  axon  activity  can  be  first  recorded  in  the 
stellate  ganglion  at  stage  28,  and  the  developmental  de- 
crease in  behavioral  delay  (Fig.  3B)  is  also  evident  in  the 
electrical  recordings  (Fig.  10A). 

Development  of  giant  axons  thus  improves  temporal 
aspects  of  jetting  performance.  Acceleration  of  a  squid 
through  water  is  a  function  of  both  intra-mantle  pressure 
and  the  rate  of  change  in  pressure  (O'Dor,  1988).  Rate  of 
change  in  mantle  diameter  is  thus  an  important  deter- 
minant of  escape  performance.  Figure  3C  plots  the  max- 
imum change  in  mantle  diameter  (data  in  Fig.  3A)  divided 
by  the  frames  to  peak  change  (data  in  Fig.  3B)  for  each 
developmental  stage.  A  sharp  rise  in  the  rate  of  mantle 
contraction  occursjust  before  hatching,  and  performance 
then  declines  towards  the  adult  level. 

Recordings  of  motor  activity  in  adult  Loligo 

In  a  previous  study  examining  the  functional  role  of 
the  giant  motor  axons  (third-order)  in  escape  jetting  (Otis 
and  Gilly,  1990),  adult  squid  were  attached  to  a  plastic 
support  platform  by  their  dorsal  mantle  surface  but  were 
otherwise  free  to  make  unrestrained  respiratory  and 
swimming  movements.  Under  these  relatively  natural 
conditions,  it  was  demonstrated  that  squid  show  appar- 
ently normal  behavioral  responses  in  regard  to  escape- 
jetting  when  compared  with  free-swimming  animals  in 
large  tanks.  The  present  study  goes  a  step  further  and 
shows  that  similar  neural  responses  can  be  obtained  with 
dissected,  pinned,  and  inverted  animals. 

Electrical  stimulation  directly  over  the  magnocellular 
lobe  in  an  adult  squid  leads  to  a  large,  short  latency 
action  potential  in  the  pallial  nerve  (2)  followed  by  a 
spike  in  an  ipsilateral  stellar  nerve  (3)  as  illustrated  in 
Figure  4A.  These  'directly'  evoked  events  are  assumed 
to  result  from  direct  activation  of  the  first-order  giant 
cells  and  represent  the  sequential  firing  of  the  second- 
and  third-order  giant  axons,  respectively,  as  described 
by  Bryant  (1959). 

The  electrode  on  the  stellar  nerve  also  picks  up  a  small 
potential  just  before  the  third-order  spike,  and  this  pre- 


almost  as  large.  (B)  No  singularly  large  axon  exists  in  the  hind-most  stellar  nerve  at  stage  26.  (C)  By  stage 
28,  a  distinctly  large  'giant'  axon  is  present  in  the  hind-most  stellar  nerve  (*).  The  fin  nerve  lies  to  the  left 
of  the  stellar  nerve  and  is  composed  entirely  of  small  axons.  (D)  The  hind-most  stellar  nerve  at  the  time  of 
hatching  shows  a  well-developed  giant  axon  (*). 


214 


W.  F.  GILLY  ET  AL. 


(1) 

E 

CO 


c 

CO 


(D 
O) 

ra 

.c 
o 


-40  -i 


-20  - 


0 


Time    to    peak 

^                                                             ^ 

i 

,       4 

• 

*      • 

Delay 
•       *       t       •       t 

Peak    r 

t    t    T    T    "     i     i     '          i     ' 

-20246 

response 


* 
Video 


Frame    Number 


Figure  2.  Escape  behavior  of  a  stage  29  Loligo  embryo  in  response  to  an  electric  shock.  (A)  Sequential 
video  frames  are  illustrated;  the  stimulus  was  applied  via  the  pipette  during  frame  0  (*).  Arrows  indicate 
mantle  diameter  (measured  at  the  widest  point).  See  text  for  additional  details.  (B)  Data  from  the  experiment 
in  Figure  2A  is  plotted  in  graphical  form,  and  the  parameters  measured  are  indicated. 


sumably  represents  the  arrival  of  the  second-order  wave 
in  the  stellate  ganglion.  The  delay  between  its  peak  and 
the  start  of  the  third-order  event  is  approximately  1  ms 
and  represents  synaptic  delay  at  the  giant  synapse.  This 
characteristic  short-latency  pattern  of  activity  in  the  stel- 
late ganglion  was  consistently  observed  in  adults  (Fig.  4A), 


hatchlings  (Fig.  4B),  and  embryos  (Fig.  6A)  only  when 
stimuli  were  applied  directly  over  the  magnocellular  lobe. 
When  the  squid  is  stimulated  by  a  light  flash  (Fig.  5A,  B) 
or  by  electrical  shocks  in  regions  other  than  the  immediate 
vicinity  of  the  first-order  giant  cells,  e.g.,  the  tentacles  (Fig. 
5C,  D),  'indirect'  responses  are  obtained.  These  occur  after 


DEVELOPMENT  OF  SQUID  ESCAPE  RESPONSE 


215 


Peak  Response 


(%  change) 


o  •-• 


"•-•- 


25   26    27    28   29  29+  29t30 
••-Embryonic  Stage *-°-*- 


i  —  i 


Days  post  Hatching 


Frames  to  Peak 


Frames  Delay 


, 
• 


-f— i — i — r 

26  28 


%  Change 
Frames  to  Peak 


1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1       I       1       I 

26  28  29»         30  2  4  6  8 

Figure  3.  Ontogeny  of  escape  jetting  in  Loligo.  Three  or  four  animals 
at  each  developmental  stage  were  studied  as  described  in  conjunction 
with  Figure  2.  and  mean  values  are  plotted.  Adult  values  are  from  data 
obtained  in  a  previous  study  (Otis  and  Gilly,  1440).  Electrical  (•,  •). 
tactile  (A),  and  photic  (O,  D)  stimuli  are  individually  plotted.  (A)  Peak 
mantle  contraction  rises  smoothly  from  stage  25  until  hatching.  Each  of 
the  stimulus  modes  yields  strong  escape  jets.  (B)  Temporal  aspects  of 
escape  performance  [frames  to  peak  (•)  and  delay  •  D)]  improve 
markedly  between  stages  28  and  30.  The  adult  value  for  delay  with  an 
electrical  stimulus  (•  over  arrow)  has  a  minimum  value  of  7-8  frames 
(Otis  and  Gilly,  1990).  (C)  Rate  of  mantle  contraction,  approximated  as 
maximum  diameter  change  divided  by  frames  to  peak,  shows  a  sharp 
increase  before  hatching  (stage  30)  and  a  post-hatching  decline  towards 
the  adult  level. 

sizable  delays  and  represent  activation  through  more 
physiological  pathways. 

General  features  of  indirectly  stimulated  motor  activity 
due  to  giant  and  non-giant  (small)  axon  pathways  ob- 
served in  the  present  study  closely  resemble  those  previ- 
ously reported  for  tethered,  intact  squid.  In  escape  re- 
sponses evoked  by  light  flashes,  the  giant  axons  fire  either 
after  a  50  ms  delay  at  the  start  of  a  burst  of  small  axon 
activity  (Figs.  5A,  B)  or  not  at  all.  In  an  indirect  response 
stimulated  by  an  electric  shock,  the  giant  axons  fire  only 
after  a  much  longer  delay  (several  hundred  ms)  and  always 
during  a  burst  of  small-axon  activity  (Figs.  5C,  D).  Giant 
axons  do  not  fire  in  every  jet  cycle,  and  the  small-axon 


B 

Figure  4.  'Direct'  responses  to  electrical  stimulation  (•)  applied  over 
the  magnocellular  lobe  in  an  adult  Loligo  (A)  and  a  7-day  post-hatching 
.Viyd'rViiV/H.s  (B).  (A)  The  electrode  on  the  pallial  nerve  (lower  trace) 
records  the  second-order  giant  axon  spike  (2)  en  passant,  while  a  second 
electrode  on  a  posterior  stellar  nerve  stump  (upper  trace)  records  the 
third-order  spike  (3)  of  the  giant  motor  axon.  (B)  A  single  electrode 
placed  over  the  stellate  ganglion  records  both  second-  and  third-order 
events.  See  text  for  additional  details. 


system  acting  alone  can  generate  intense  episodes  of  motor 
activity  (second  cycle  in  Fig.  5C).  In  all  these  respects  our 
findings  agree  with  Otis  and  Gilly  ( 1990). 

Recordings  of  motor  outputs  in  hatchling 
and  embryonic  Loligo 

Recordings  from  the  stellate  ganglion  of  late-stage  em- 
bryos and  hatchlings  after  electrical  stimulation  of  the 
head  show  activity  associated  with  both  giant  and  non- 


—^ 

400  uV 
1000  uV 


5  ms  i  ,  3 


400  uV 
600  uV 


A 


Figure  5.  Bursts  of  motor  activity  associated  with  escape  jetting  in 
adult  Loligo.  In  each  panel  (A-D)  the  upper  trace  is  from  the  pallial 
nerve,  and  the  lower  trace  is  from  a  stellar  nerve.  (A)  A  light  flash  stimulus 
(*)  results  in  firing  of  the  giant  axons  (2  and  3)  after  a  delay  of  ~50  ms 
and  before  the  onset  of  the  non-giant  motor  burst.  (B)  Portions  ot  the 
records  in  (A)  are  displayed  on  an  expanded  time  scale  to  illustrate  the 
time  course  of  the  giant  axon  spikes.  (C)  An  electrical  stimulus  (•)  leads 
to  giant  axon  activation  after  a  long  delay  (>500  ms).  In  this  case,  the 
burst  of  small  axon  motor  activity  commences  before  the  giant  spike.  A 
second  escape  cycle  at  the  end  of  the  record  is  driven  by  non-giant  axons 
acting  alone  and  shows  no  giant  spike.  (D)  The  first  cycle  in  (C)  is  dis- 
played at  an  expanded  time  base. 


216 


W.  F.  GILLY  ET  AL. 


giant  motor  systems.  Figure  6A  illustrates  such  recordings 
from  a  stage  29+  embryo.  Giant  fiber  excitation  is  in- 
dicated by  an  initial  small,  negative-going  spike  (2)  fol- 
lowed by  a  larger  positive  event  (3).  As  discussed  above, 
the  first  component  (2)  represents  arrival  of  the  impulse 
in  the  second-order  giant  fiber  entering  the  stellate  gan- 
glion via  the  pallial  nerve,  whereas  the  second  component 
(3)  reflects  the  summed  action  potentials  from  proximal 
parts  of  the  third-order  giants  lying  within  the  ganglion. 
Thus,  moving  the  recording  electrode  anteriorly  along  the 
pallial  nerve  amplifies  the  second-order  event  and  elim- 
inates the  third  (Fig.  6B),  whereas  moving  the  electrode 
posteriorly  along  the  larger  stellar  nerves  isolates  activity 
in  the  third-order  giant  fiber  (Fig.  6C).  Moving  the  elec- 
trode peripherally  along  one  of  the  smaller  (anterior)  stellar 
nerves  out  into  the  muscle  field  shows  the  third-order  spike 
followed  by  a  large  muscle  potential  (m.  Fig.  6D). 

Following  stimulation  over  the  brachial  ganglion,  ac- 
tivity can  be  recorded  at  all  three  stages  in  the  giant  fiber 
pathway  by  placing  one  recording  electrode  over  the  mag- 
nocellular  lobe  and  a  second  on  the  stellate  ganglion.  The 
first  electrode  records  the  first-order  giant  spike  ( 1 )  while 
the  other  records  the  second-  and  third-order  events  in 
sequence  (Figs.  7A,  B). 

Firing  of  the  first-order  cell  is  invariably  followed  by 
activation  of  the  other  two  elements,  and  the  delay  from 
the  peak  of  the  first-order  spike  to  the  start  of  the  third- 
order  event  is  only  2.0-2.5  ms.  The  delay  that  precedes 
firing  of  the  first-order  spike  during  an  indirect  response 
is  much  longer  (e.g..  26  ms  in  Fig.  7A  or  13  ms  in  Fig. 


Figure  6.  Electrical  activity  in  the  second-  and  third-order  giant  axons 
in  a  stage  29+  Loligo  embryo.  Direct  responses  of  the  giant  fiber  pathway 
were  generated  by  stimulation  over  the  magnocellular  lobe.  (A)  The  re- 
cording electrode  was  placed  directly  over  the  stellate  ganglion,  and  the 
incoming  second-order  spike  (2)  and  the  out-going  third-order  wave  (3) 
are  recorded.  (B)  The  electrode  was  positioned  on  the  pallial  nerve  just 
proximal  to  the  stellate  ganglion,  and  the  second-order  spike  is  thereby 
isolated.  (C)  The  electrode  was  placed  on  the  emergence  of  the  posterior 
stellar  nerves  from  the  ganglion;  this  isolates  activity  in  the  third-order 
giant  axons.  (D)  The  electrode  was  placed  over  an  anterior  stellar  nerve 
in  the  muscle  field  of  the  mantle.  This  reveals  the  third-order  giant  axon 
spike  and  a  large  muscle  potential  (m). 


Figure  7.  Timing  of  electrical  activity  from  all  three  stages  of  the 
giant  fiber  pathway  following  indirect  electrical  stimulation  over  the  bra- 
chial ganglion  in  a  stage  29++  (A)  or  30  (B)  Loligo.  In  each  panel  the 
lower  trace  is  recorded  with  an  electrode  on  the  magnocellular  lobe,  and 
a  composite  event  including  the  first-order  giant  spike  is  obtained  (1). 
Upper  traces  are  recorded  from  the  stellate  ganglion,  where  second-  (2) 
and  third-order  (3)  events  are  detected. 


7B)  and  presumably  reflects  'processing'  time  in  the  central 
nervous  system.  Small  potentials  precede  the  initiation  of 
the  first-order  spike  in  Figure  7B  and  must  represent 
summed  activity  in  pathways  leading  into  the  magnocel- 
lular lobe.  There  is  also  generally  an  outburst  of  small- 
unit  activity  coincident  with  and  following  the  first-order 
giant  spike,  which  itself  appears  difficult  to  resolve  except 
for  the  rising  phase  ( 1  in  Fig.  7B). 

Indirectly  stimulated  escape  responses  in  hatchlings 
produce  the  pattern  of  second-  and  third-order  giant  spikes 
discussed  above  (Figs.  8 A,  B),  and  the  subsequent  activity 
in  the  small  motor  axons  takes  the  form  of  flurries  of 
irregular,  compound  action  potentials  (Fig.  8 A).  Unit  ac- 
tivity is  generally  difficult  to  resolve,  and  these  potentials 
probably  represent  the  firing  of  dozens  of  axons  in  rough 
synchrony.  Although  these  compound  events  may  ap- 
proach third-order  giant  spikes  in  amplitude,  they  are 
readily  distinguished  from  the  latter  by  their  irregular  and 
variable  waveforms  and  slower  rise  times.  Bursts  of  small 
unit  activity  can  be  elicited  by  weak  stimulation  without 
excitation  of  the  giant  fiber  system  (upper  trace  in  Fig. 
8A)  and  would  be  associated  in  nature  with  non-giant 
escape  jetting,  as  occurs  in  adults. 

Two  consistent  and  striking  features  characterize  neural 
recordings  of  indirectly  stimulated  escape  responses  in 
late-stage  embryos  and  hatchlings  and  clearly  differentiate 
them  from  analogous  records  obtained  in  adults.  First, 
the  minimum  latency  for  excitation  of  the  third-order 
giant  axons  in  hatchlings  by  photic  stimulation  is  consid- 
erably shorter  than  that  in  adults  (~15  ms  in  Figs.  8B, 
9F  vs.  50  ms  for  adults  in  Fig.  5A),  and  the  latency  fol- 
lowing electrical  stimulation  in  hatchlings  can  be  nearly 
as  brief  (Fig.  8A).  In  adult  animals,  this  latency  is  at  least 
several  hundred  ms  (Fig.  5C;  c.f.  Otis  and  Gilly,  1990). 

A  second  and  more  pronounced  difference  is  that  when 
the  third-order  giants  fire  in  hatchlings,  their  impulses 
arise  in  the  stellate  ganglion  several  ms  before  those  of 
the  small  motor  axons.  This  is  true  regardless  of  whether 
stimulation  is  via  electric  shocks  (Fig.  8A)  or  light  flash 
(Fig.  8B).  This  pattern  of  giant  versus  non-giant  activity 


DEVELOPMENT  OF  SQUID  ESCAPE  RESPONSE 


217 


"^     "'    '!•"        *'\«A.v~w>'" 


A  B 

Figure  8.  Companson  of  motor  outputs  from  a  stage  30  Loligo  in 
response  to  an  electrical  shock  at  the  base  of  the  tentacles  (•.  A)  and  to 
a  light  flash  (*,  B).  (A)  Upper  trace  shows  activity  of  small  axon  system 
acting  alone;  stimulus  was  sub-threshold  for  giant  fiber  activation.  Lower 
trace  was  recorded  with  a  slightly  stronger  shock,  which  led  to  indirect 
excitation  of  the  giant  fiber  pathway.  The  giant  axon  spike  arrives  in  the 
stellate  ganglion  before  the  small  axon  activity.  ( B)  A  light  flash  produces 
a  similar  pattern  of  motor  activity  in  which  the  small  axon  wave  follows 
the  giant  fiber  response. 

for  electrically  stimulated  responses  is  thus  temporally  in- 
verted in  comparison  to  the  picture  in  adults,  where  giant 
axon  spikes  always  fire  50-75  ms  after  the  onset  of  the 
burst  of  non-giant  activity  (Fig.  5C,  D:  Otis  and  Gilly, 
1990). 

Development  of  motor  patterns  prior  to  hatching 

Small  axon  control  of  jetting  behavior  is  demonstrable 
at  stage  26  (the  earliest  examined).  Intermittent  bouts  of 
spontaneous  rhythmic  activity  occur  and  represent  normal 
respiratory  cycles.  Electrical  stimulation  evokes  bursts  of 
small  unit  activity  resembling  these  respiratory  bursts,  but 
stimulated  activity  is  generally  more  intense  and  long- 
lasting  (Fig.  9A).  Latency  for  small  axon  excitation  appears 
to  be  brief  at  this  stage,  but  this  may  reflect  direct  stim- 
ulation of  these  pathways  in  the  small  embryos  at  this 
early  stage.  Small  axon  responses  continue  with  little 
change  in  the  above  pattern  throughout  subsequent  de- 
velopmental stages,  except  for  an  apparent  increase  in 
latency  to  30-50  ms  by  stages  27-28  (Fig.  9C). 

Light  flashes  are  ineffective  at  triggering  escape  jets  or 
neural  activity  in  the  stellar  nerves  at  stages  26  (Fig.  9B) 
or  27  (not  illustrated).  Photic  stimulation  does  produce 
bursts  of  small  axon  activity  in  the  stellate  ganglion  by 
stage  28  (Fig.  9D).  coincident  with  anatomical  maturation 
of  the  eye  (Segawa  et  ai,  1988). 

Giant  axon  responses  can  first  be  evoked  by  electrical 
stimuli  at  stage  28  in  embryonic  squid  (Fig.  9E)  and  by 
light  flashes  around  the  time  of  hatching  (Fig.  9F).  Gen- 
erally, the  picture  of  functional  development  of  the  giant 
fiber  system  from  stage  28  through  hatching  is  one  of 
progressive  maturation,  in  terms  of  speeding  of  the  action 
potential  waveform,  reduced  response  latencies,  and  de- 
creased synaptic  delay.  The  immaturity  of  the  giant  fiber 
pathway  at  stages  28-29  is  suggested  by:  (i)  the  small  am- 
plitudes and  long  durations  of  both  second-  and  third- 
order  giant  spikes;  (ii)  the  relatively  long  response  la- 
tency— the  second-order  spike  takes  more  than  20  ms  to 
arrive  in  the  stellate  ganglion  compared  with  a  delay  of 


-12  ms  at  stages  29++  or  30  (Fig.  10A),  and  (iii)  the 
progressive  decrease  for  second-  to  third-order  synaptic 
transmission  during  development  (Fig.  10B).  These 
changes  parallel  the  improvement  in  behavioral  perfor- 
mance as  indicated  by  the  square  symbols  and  dashed 
curve  in  Figure  10A.  which  are  behavioral  data  replotted 
from  Figure  3B. 

Post-hatching  development  of  giant  and  non-giant 
motor  patterns 

Although  the  embryonic  pattern  of  giant  axon  use  is 
evident  at  the  time  of  hatching,  several  distinctive  features 
of  the  adult  motor  patterns  (Otis  and  Gilly,  1990)  may 
emerge  shortly  thereafter.  Figure  1 1 A  shows  stellar  nerve 
discharge  of  a  2-week-old  Sepioteuthis  in  response  to  an 
electric  shock  applied  to  the  tentacles.  A  burst  of  small 
unit  activity  follows  the  stimulus  at  short  latency;  this  is 
a  pattern  typical  of  embryonic  and  hatchling  Loligo.  The 
lower  trace  is  plotted  at  an  expanded  time  base.  A  stronger 
shock  produced  the  response  pictured  in  Figure  1 1  B.  A 
short  latency,  non-giant  burst  again  occurs,  but  in  this 
case  it  is  followed  by  a  giant  axon  spike  after  80  ms.  Tim- 
ing of  giant  axon  activation  in  relation  to  that  of  the  small 
axons  is  adult-like  (Fig.  5D),  although  the  overall  latency 


?00  ^jV 


Stage    26 


IJ^T  ^~w-^ 

Slage    28 


100      200  jjV 


Slage    30 


Figure  9.  Functional  maturation  of  the  giant  fiber  pathway  in  em- 
bryonic Loligo.  Indirect  electrical  (•)  or  light  flash  (*)  stimuli  were  used 
to  elicit  escape  responses  at  various  stages  of  development;  all  recordings 
illustrated  were  obtained  from  the  stellate  ganglion.  (A)  At  stage  26. 
electrical  stimulation  elicits  only  small  axon  activity.  (B)  A  light  flash  is 
ineffective  at  stimulating  escape  responses  or  producing  any  detectable 
motor  outputs  at  stage  26.  (C)  Giant  axon  activity  is  still  not  produced 
by  electrical  stimuli  at  stage  27.  (D)  Photic  stimulation  produces  small 
axon  activity  but  no  giant  spikes  at  stage  28.  (E)  Giant  axon  responses 
are  first  detectable  at  stage  28  with  electrical  stimuli.  (F)  A  light  flash 
stimulus  at  stage  30  produces  a  giant  axon  response.  See  text  for  additional 
details. 


218 


W.  F.  GILLY  I.I    I/ 


50  —I 


Latency 
(ms) 


>      2, 

\    ~. 


i — rn — i    i    i    i — i — r~r 

26     27     28      29    29+  29++ 30      1        2       3 


Embryonic 
Stage 


Days  post 
Hatching 


B 


20  — 


Synaptic 
Delay 

(ms)       1.0- 


13 


I          I          I  I          I          I          I          I          I         I 

26     27     28     29    29+  29++  30     1        2        3 


Embryonic 
Stage 


Days  post 
Hatching 


Figure  10.  Improvement  in  performance  of  the  giant  fiber  pathway 
during  late  embryogenesis  in  /  oligo.  (A)  Response  latency  (time  to  sec- 
ond-order giant  spike)  decreases  between  stage  28  (time  of  first  detectable 
giant  axon  activity)  and  30  (hatching).  Filled  circles  were  obtained  with 
indirect  electrical  stimuli;  open  symbol  was  obtained  with  light  flashes. 
Mean  values  and  the  number  of  experiments  are  given;  standard  error 
of  the  mean  is  smaller  than  the  symbols.  Squares  and  dashed  curve  are 
behavioral  data  which  has  been  replotted  from  Figure  3B.  (B)  Synaptic 
delay  between  the  second-  and  third-order  giants  (across  the  giant  synapse 
in  the  stellate  ganglion)  also  decreases  between  stages  28  and  30.  Means, 
number  of  experiments,  and  standard  error  are  indicated. 

is  much  less  than  that  in  adults  (Fig.  5C).  The  motor  pat- 
tern underlying  the  escape  response  in  Figure  1  IB  thus 
shows  both  embryonic  and  adult  qualities. 

More  complex  escape  responses  with  the  long  delays 
characteristic  of  the  adult  can  also  be  generated  shortly 
after  hatching  in  some  animals.  Results  from  another  Se- 
pioteuthis (~5  days  post-hatching)  are  shown  in  Figure 
12.  Delayed  escape  jets  driven  by  only  the  small  axon 
system  (Fig.  1 2A)  and  by  both  small  and  giant  axons  acting 
in  concert  (Fig.  12B)  are  well  developed.  Adult-like  mul- 
tiple cycle  responses  showing  both  of  the  above  types  of 
motor  patterns  are  also  evident  (Fig.  1 2C).  When  the  giant 
axons  are  used  during  such  delayed  escape  responses,  their 
firing  is  timed  to  occur  after  the  non-giant  system  initiates 
the  jet  cycle.  In  every  respect,  the  motor  patterns  in  Figure 
12  are  adult-like.  We  do  not  yet  know  precisely  when  the 
adult-like  motor  patterns  appear  in  Sepioteuthis  after 
hatching,  but  stage  29-29++  embryos  display  only  the 
fast-start  embryonic  pattern  seen  in  Loligo  (data  not  il- 
lustrated). 

In  addition  to  the  delayed  bouts  of  motor  activity  in 
Figure  12,  an  initial  brief  burst  of  activity  occurs  with  a 
delay  of  ~20  ms  in  every  case  illustrated.  The  origin  of 


this  activity  is  presently  unknown,  and  recordings  in  adult 
squid  also  reveal  similar  short-latency  activity  in  stellar 
nerves  following  an  electrical  stimulus  (cf.  Fig.  3  in  Otis 
and  Gilly,  1990).  In  the  latter  case,  there  is  no  detectable 
short-latency  mantle  contraction.  Comparable  behavioral 
experiments  have  not  been  carried  out  with  juvenile  Se- 
piotheuthis. 

Discussion 

Motor  control  of  escape  behavior  in  late-stage  embryos 
and  hatchlings  shows  important  similarities  to  the  cor- 
responding situation  in  adult  squid.  In  both  cases,  escape 
jetting  is  under  the  influence  of  two  parallel  motor  path- 
ways: the  giant  fiber  system  and  a  small  axon  (non-giant) 
system.  Strong  escape  jets  can  be  driven  by  the  small- 
axon  system  acting  alone,  with  no  giant  fiber  involvement 
whatsoever  (Figs.  9C,  8 A).  The  small  axon  pathway  is  the 
first  to  develop  anatomically  and  become  functional  dur- 
ing embryonic  development,  and  vigorous  escape  jets  are 
possible  before  the  giant  axons  appear. 

Giant  axon  excitation  provides  a  potent  boost  to  the 
small  axon  system,  however,  and  this  greatly  improves 
escape  jetting  performance.  This  was  directly  demon- 
strated in  adult  animals  (Otis  and  Gilly,  1990).  It  is  also 
evident  in  embryonic  development  (stages  28-30)  as  a 
marked  decrease  in  response  latency  [seen  behaviorally 


J, 


B 


Figure  II.  Partial  development  of  adult-like  use  of  the  giant  fiber 
pathway  in  a  14-day  post-hatching  Sepioteuthis.  (A)  Upper  trace  was 
recorded  from  the  stellate  ganglion  following  an  electrical  stimulus  de- 
livered to  the  base  of  the  tentacles  (•).  Small  unit  activity  only  is  gen- 
erated, and  the  latency  is  brief.  Lower  trace  shows  the  initial  portion  of 
the  response  following  the  shock  displayed  at  an  expanded  time  scale. 
(B)  A  stronger  shock  to  the  same  site  as  in  (A)  produces  a  stronger  burst 
of  small  axon  activity  at  a  short  latency  and  a  single  giant  axon  spike  in 
the  middle  of  this  burst.  Lower  trace  shows  the  expanded  version  to 
identify  the  giant  fiber  spike.  The  occurrence  of  the  giant  spike  well  alter 
the  onset  of  the  small  unit  burst  is  thus  adult-like,  but  the  brief  latency 
(<20  ms)  for  the  non-giant  burst  is  characteristic  of  embryos. 


DEVELOPMENT  OF  SQUID  ESCAPE  RESPONSE 


219 


7*jl^v«X>W, 


Figure  12.  Fully  developed  adult-like  interplay  of  giant  and  non- 
giant  motor  systems  in  escape  responses  of  a  5-day  post-hatching  Sc/n- 
Kti'iilhis.  (A)  An  electrical  stimulus  (•)  to  the  side  of  the  head  produces 
an  escape  jet  at  a  latency  of  —500  ms  that  is  driven  only  by  small  motor 
axons.  (B)  A  stronger  stimulus  to  the  same  site  produces  a  delayed  escape 
jet  driven  by  both  small  axons  and  giant  axons  acting  in  concert.  Non- 
giant  activity  precedes  giant  axon  activation.  Lower  trace  shows  an  ex- 
pansion of  the  record  around  the  giant  fiber  spike.  (C)  Stimulation  on 
the  ventral  surface  of  the  head  just  anterior  to  the  eye  produces  a  multiple 
cycle  escape  response  at  a  long  latency.  The  first  cycle  is  driven  by  the 
small  axon  motor  system  acting  alone,  whereas  the  second  also  involves 
the  giant  axon  pathway. 


(Fig.  3B)  or  neurophysiologically  (Fig.  10A)],  time  to  peak 
mantle  contraction  (Fig.  3B),  and  rate  of  contraction  (Fig. 
3C).  The  time  course  of  this  improvement,  beginning  at 
stage  28-29,  coincides  closely  with  the  anatomical  ap- 
pearance and  maturation  of  the  third-order  giant  motor 
axons  in  the  more  posterior  stellar  nerves  that  control 
jetting.  Performance  peaks  at  hatching.  Presumably  this 
is  a  valuable  capability,  because  the  embryonic  squid  must 
jet  vigorously  to  escape  from  the  confines  of  the  egg  mass 
in  order  to  hatch. 

At  present  it  is  not  known  how  the  neural  portions  of 
these  giant  and  non-giant  motor  systems  are  associated 
with  the  two  types  of  circular  muscle  fibers  in  adult  squid 
mantle  (Bone  el  at..  1981;  Mommsen  el  ai,  1981).  De- 
veloping embryos  and  hatchlings  would  provide  a  valuable 
system  in  which  to  pursue  this  question,  because  the  two 
motor  systems  do  not  develop  in  perfect  synchrony.  We 
have  not  carried  out  an  anatomical  analysis  of  the  muscle 
fiber  types  in  these  young  animals. 

Although  the  overall  picture  of  dual  motor  control  of 


escape  jetting  is  similar  in  embryos,  hatchlings,  and  adults, 
details  of  how  the  two  motor  systems  are  employed  when 
they  act  together  are  strikingly  different.  In  embryos  and 
hatchlings,  activity  in  the  third-order  giant  axons  always 
precedes  the  burst  of  small  unit  motor  activity  by  several 
ms.  This  is  true  regardless  of  whether  stimulation  is  by 
electric  shock  or  light  flash.  Latency  to  firing  for  the  giant 
axon  varies  from  ~40  to  10  ms,  depending  on  the  exact 
stage  of  development  (Fig.  10  A).  This  delay  basically  rep- 
resents the  time  required  to  excite  the  first-order  giant  cell 
(Fig.  7).  Once  this  cell  fires,  the  second-  and  third-order 
giants  follow  within  2-3  ms.  In  embryos  and  hatchlings, 
all  escape  responses  are  thus  of  the  fast-start  variety,  al- 
though the  latency  is  never  so  brief  as  with  artificial  direct 
stimulation  of  the  first-order  giant  (Fig.  4). 

In  our  experiments  on  adult  squid,  only  sudden  visual 
stimuli  (e.g.  light  flash)  have  been  effective  in  producing 
a  fast-start  pattern  of  giant  axon  excitation  (Fig.  5A)  like 
that  seen  in  embryos  or  hatchlings,  where  giant  axon  firing 
precedes  the  burst  of  non-giant  activity.  Latency  for  giant 
axon  activation,  even  in  this  fastest  case,  is  much  longer 
than  that  in  hatchlings  (50  ms  versus  10  ms)  and  presum- 
ably again  represents  the  time  before  the  first-order  giant 
fires. 

Electrical  stimuli  in  adults  produce  a  second  pattern  of 
giant  axon  use  that  is  not  seen  in  the  embryo.  This  delayed- 
escape  mode  shows  a  minimum  latency  of  several  hundred 
ms  and,  more  significantly,  a  burst  of  non-giant  motor 
output  that  commences  50-100  ms  before  the  giant  axons 
fire  (Fig.  5C).  The  small-axon  system  thus  appears  to  be 
the  primary  effecter  of  this  type  of  escape  jet,  and  the 
giant  axons  are  booster  elements  that  are  optionally  re- 
cruited during  any  given  cycle  of  a  delayed-type  response. 

Otis  and  Gilly  (1990)  have  argued  that  a  great  deal  of 
complex  processing  in  the  central  nervous  system  under- 
lies effective  use  of  the  giant  fiber  pathway  in  this  delayed- 
escape  mode.  This  idea  is  in  consonance  with  results  on 
embryonic  and  hatchling  Loligo  presented  in  this  paper. 
Much  of  the  brain  at  these  stages  is  not  yet  differentiated 
(Young,  1939;  Martin,  1965),  and  these  animals  do  not 
yet  have  the  capability  of  orchestrating  complex  escape 
behavior,  despite  the  functional  presence  of  all  the  nec- 
essary peripheral  motor  components.  Presumably,  devel- 
opment of  higher  order  neural  centers  is  necessary  to  co- 
ordinate the  interplay  of  giant  and  non-giant  motor  sys- 
tems. 

When  and  how  does  the  strictly  fast-start  embryonic 
pattern  in  escape  jetting  evolve  into  the  complex  capa- 
bilities of  the  adult?  This  is  not  yet  clear  in  Loligo.  We 
have  seen  no  indication  of  delayed  escape  responses  in 
either  behavioral  or  neurophysiological  experiments  for 
up  to  6  days  post-hatching.  Preliminary  work  on  post- 
hatching  Sepioteuthis  is  also  described  in  this  paper.  In 
this  case,  perfectly  normal,  adult-like  firing  patterns  of  the 
giant  and  non-giant  motor  systems  were  observed  in  an- 


220 


W.  F.  GILLY  ET  AL. 


imals  only  5  days  old  (Fig.  1 2 ),  and  other  animals  showed 
a  curious  mixture  of  embryonic  and  adult-like  character- 
istics (Fig.  1 1). 

The  specimens  of  Sepi< >teuth is  that  we  studied  differed 
from  our  Loligo  subjects  in  two  important  respects.  First. 
Sepioteuthis  is  a  much  larger  and  possibly  more  developed 
(i.e.,  neurologically)  animal  at  birth.  Second,  the  Sepi- 
oteuthis had  been  actively  feeding  and  growing  for  the 
entire  time  after  hatching  (5-21  days),  whereas  Loligo 
was  provided  with  no  (for  the  neurophysiological  work) 
or  minimal  food  (for  the  behavioral  work)  and  was  main- 
tained for  no  more  than  6  days.  Either  or  both  of  these 
factors  could  be  relevant  to  the  early  attainment  of  co- 
ordinated delayed-escape  jetting  in  young  Sepioteuthis. 
In  the  first  case,  Sepioteuthis  may  simply  develop  the 
adult-like  motor  patterns  more  quickly  after  birth  than 
does  Loligo.  The  second  possibility  is  more  intriguing, 
however. 

Hatchling  squid  have  limited  energy  reserves  sufficient 
only  for  several  days  (O'Dor  el  ai.  1986)  during  which 
time  the  animals  must  learn  to  capture  prey  or  face  star- 
vation (Hurley.  1976;  Yang  et  ai.  1983.  1986).  Because 
prey  items  consist  largely  of  fast-moving  copepods.  and 
high  speed  pursuit  is  involved  in  prey-capture,  it  would 
seem  advantageous  to  employ  the  giant  fiber  pathway  in 
this  activity.  To  do  this  effectively,  however,  strict  control 
over  excitation  of  the  first  order  giants  must  be  necessary 
to  provide  the  giant  axon-mediated  boost  precisely  at  the 
correct  moment.  This  capability — critically  timed  exci- 
tation of  the  giant  fiber  pathway — is  the  basic  feature  of 
the  adult  delayed-escape  response  that  differentiates  it 
from  the  fast-start  response.  The  possibility  that  such  a 
profound  change-over  in  the  pattern  of  giant  axon  use  in 
hatchling  squid  might  be  associated  with  the  perfection 
of  feeding  behavior  is  a  prospect  that  we  are  currently 
pursuing. 

Acknowledgments 

We  thank  Natasha  Fraley  and  Patricia  Gosling  for  per- 
forming the  behavioral  experiments,  and  Dr.  Roger  Han- 
Ion's  group  at  U.T.M.B.,  Galveston,  Texas,  for  providing 
living  Sepioteuthis.  This  work  was  supported  by  grants 
from  the  Whitehall  Foundation  (J86-1 10)  the  Office  of 
Naval  Research  (N00014-89-J-1744)  and  the  N.I.H.  (NS- 
17510). 

Literature  Cited 

Bone,  Q.,  A.  Pulsford,  and  A.  D.  Chubb.  1981.     Squid  mantle  muscle. 
J.  Mar.  Bio/.  Assoc.  L'.A'.  61:  327-342. 


Boyle,  P.  R.  1986.     Neural  control  of  cephalopod  behavior.  Pp.  1-97 
in  The  Mullusca.  vol.  9,  pan  2.  A.  O.  D.  Willows,  ed.  Academic 
Press,  New  York. 
Bryant,  S.  H.  1959.     The  function  of  the  proximal  synapses  of  the  squid 

stellate  ganglion.  J.  Gen.  Physio/.  42:  609-616. 
Eaton,  R.  C.  1984.  ed.     h'eural  Mechanisms  ol  Startle  Behavior.  Plenum. 

New  York. 

Hurley,  A.  C.  1976.     Feeding  behavior,  food  consumption,  growth,  and 
respiration  of  the  squid  Loligo  opalescens  raised  in  the  laboratory. 
Fish.  Bull  74:  176-182. 
Mackie,  G.  O.  1990.     Giant  axons  and  control  of  jetting  in  the  squid 

Loligo  and  the  jellyfish  Ag/anlha.  Can.  J  Zoo/.  68:  1421-1431. 
Marthy,  H.-J.  1987.     Ontogenesis  of  the  nervous  system  in  cephalopods. 
Pp.  443-459  in  Nervous  Systems  in  Invertebrates,  M.  A.  Ali,  ed. 
Plenum.  New  York. 

Martin,  R.  1965.     On  the  structure  and  embryonic  development  of  the 
giant  fibre  system  of  the  squid  Loligo  vulgaris.  Z  Zellforsch.  67:  77- 
85. 
Martin,  R.  1969.     The  structural  organization  of  the  intracerebral  giant 

fiber  system  of  cephalopods.  Z  Zellforsch.  97:  50-68. 
Martin,  R.  K.  1977.     The  giant  nerve  fibre  system  of  cephalopds.  Recent 

structural  findings.  Symp.  Zoo/.  Soc  Loud.  38:  261-275.443. 
Messenger,  J.  B.  1983.     Multimodal  convergence  and  the  regulation 

of  motor  programs  in  cephalopods.  Fortschr.  Zoo/  28:  77-97. 
Mommesen,  I.  P.,  J.  Ballantyne,  D.  MacDonald,  J.  Gosline,  and  P.  \V. 
Hochachka.  1981.     Analogues  of  red  and  white  muscle  in  squid 
mantle.  Proc.  Nail.  Acad.  Sci.  L'SA  78:  3274-3278. 
O'Dor,  R.  K.  1988.     The  forces  acting  on  swimming  squid.  J.  E\p.  Biol. 

137:421-442. 

O'Dor,  R.  K.,  E.  A.  Foy,  P.  A.  Helm,  and  N.  Balch.  1986.  The  loco- 
motion and  energetics  of  hatchling  squid.  Ilex  illecebrosus.  Am.  Ma- 
lawi Bull.  4:  55-60. 

Otis,  T.  S.,  and  W.  F.  Gilly.  1990.     Jet-propelled  escape  in  the  squid 
Loligo  opalescent:  concerted  control  by  giant  and  non-giant  motor 
axon  pathways.  Proc.  Nad.  Acad.  Sci.  USA  87:  291 1-2915. 
Packard,  A.  1969.     Jet  propulsion  and  the  giant  fibre  response  of  Loligo. 

Nature  221:  875-877. 
Prosser,  C.  L.,  and  J.  Z.  Young.  1937.     Responses  of  muscles  of  the 

squid  to  repetitive  stimulation.  Biol.  Bull  73:  237-241. 
Segawa,    S.,    \V.    T.    Yang,    H.-J.    Marthy,    and    R.     I.    Hanlon. 
1988.     Illustrated  embryonic  stages  of  the  eastern  Atlantic  squid  Lo- 
ligo forbesi.  I'eligeriQ:  230-243. 
Wilson,  D.  1960.     Nervous  control  of  movement  in  cephalopods.  J. 

Exp.  Biol.  37:  57-72. 

Yang,  \V.  T.,  R.  F.  Hixon,  P.  E.  Turk,  M.  E.  Krejci,  W.  H.  Hulet,  and 
R.  T.  Hanlon.  1986.  Growth,  behavior,  and  sexual  maturation  of 
the  market  squid,  Loligo  opalescens,  cultured  through  the  life  cycle. 
Fish.  fiii/A  84:771-798. 

Yang,  W.  T.,  R.  T.  Hanlon,  M.  E.  Krejci,  R.  F.  Hixon,  and  \V.  H.  Hulet. 
1983.  Laboratory  rearing  of  Loligo  opalescens.  the  market  squid 
of  California.  Aquacullure  31:  77-88. 

Young,  J.  Z.  1939.  Fused  neurons  and  synaptic  contacts  in  the  giant 
nerve  fibres  of  cephalopods.  Philas.  Trans.  R.  Soc.  Loud.  Ser.  B  229: 
465-503. 

Young,  J.  Z.  1938.  The  functioning  of  the  giant  nerve  fibres  of  the 
squid../.  E.\p.  Biol.  15:  170-185. 


Reference:  Bid.  Bull  180:  221-227.  (April.  1991) 


Factors  Affecting  the  Sensory  Response  Characteristics 

of  the  Cephalopod  Statocyst  and  their  Relevance 

in  Predicting  Swimming  Performance 

RODDY  WILLIAMSON 

The  Marine  Biological  Association,  Citadel  Hill,  Plymouth  PL  I  2PB,  England 


Abstract.  The  statocyst  in  cephalopods  is  the  main  organ 
of  balance  and  operates  in  a  manner  similar  to  the  ves- 
tihular  system  of  vertebrates.  This  paper  reviews  the  prin- 
cipal factors  affecting  the  sensitivity  and  frequency  re- 
sponse of  the  statocyst.  These  include  morphological  fea- 
tures, such  as  the  size  and  shape  of  the  statocyst,  its  canal 
structure,  and  the  size  of  the  cupulae  and  maculae,  as  well 
as  physiological  features,  such  as  the  electrotonic  coupling 
of  sensory  cells,  the  impact  of  the  efterents,  and  the  mo- 
tility  of  some  cells.  The  use  of  statocyst  characteristics  in 
predicting  the  locomotory  performance  of  different  ceph- 
alopod  species  is  discussed. 

Introduction 

For  spatial  control  of  locomotion,  an  animal  needs  in- 
formation about  its  orientation  with  respect  to  gravity 
and  its  motion  relative  to  its  surroundings.  This  infor- 
mation could  be  derived  from  a  variety  of  sensory  systems, 
ranging  from  vision  to  electroreception,  but  most  animals 
have  developed  specific  sense  organs  responding  to  linear 
and  angular  accelerations;  e.g.,  the  vestibular  system  in 
vertebrates  and  the  statocysts  of  cephalopods.  It  has  been 
proposed  that  the  sensory  response  characteristics  of  these 
receptor  systems  are  matched  to  their  likely  inputs;  i.e.. 
that  the  frequency  response  range  and  sensitivity  of  the 
system  reflect  the  accelerations  imposed  by  an  animal's 
own  movements  (Jones  and  Spells,  1963;  Jones,  1984). 
Thus  the  vestibular  system  of  a  small  agile  animal  (e.g.. 
a  bird)  is  more  sensitive  to  higher  frequencies  of  move- 
ment than  that  of  a  slower  moving  animal  living  in  a 
denser  medium  (e.g.,  a  fish)  (Correia  et  a/..  1981).  This 
idea  can  also  be  applied  to  cephalopods  where,  by  looking 
at  the  statocysts,  we  can  try  to  predict  what  kind  of  lo- 

Received  22  August  1990;  accepted  27  November  1990. 


comotion  is  used  by  the  animal  (Maddock  and  Young. 
1984;  Morris,  1988;  Young,  1989).  This  is  a  particularly 
valuable  approach  to  animals  with  unstudied  lifestyles. 
This  paper  reviews  some  of  the  evidence  for  this  propo- 
sition and  identifies  some  of  the  features  that  are  likely 
to  influence  the  sensitivity  and  response  characteristics  of 
cephalopod  statocysts. 

The  parameters  affecting  statocyst  sensitivity  can  be 
divided  into  two  main  areas,  morphological  features  and 
physiological  features.  For  convenience  these  are  consid- 
ered separately,  but  of  course,  they  act  in  concert  within 
the  living  animal. 

Morphological  Features 

The  general  morphology  of  each  of  the  paired  right  and 
left  statocysts  is  a  fluid-filled  cavity  within  the  cranial  car- 
tilage (Fig.  1).  The  statocyst  itself  varies  considerably  in 
shape  in  different  cephalopods;  in  Octopus  (Fig.  1A)  it  is 
almost  spherical,  whereas  in  \  'ampyrotenthis  it  is  short, 
wide,  and  shallow  (Young,  1960,  1989).  Many  statocysts 
have  sac-like  protrusions  into  the  surrounding  cartilage 
(Stephens  and  Young,  1982),  or  cartilaginous  pegs  or 
hooks  (the  anticristae  and  hamuli)  that  project  into  the 
statocyst  interior  (Fig.  IB);  these  projections  presumably 
constrict  or  direct  the  flow  of  the  endolymph.  Again,  this 
can  vary  from  the  single  anticrista  in  Octopus,  to  the  38 
anticristae  and  5  hamuli  in  Egea  (Young,  1984). 

Each  statocyst  has  two  main  areas  of  receptor  epithe- 
lium (Fig.  1 ).  The  first  is  a  macula  or  plate  of  sensory  hair 
cells  with  an  overlying  statolith.  All  coleoids  have  a  macula 
carrying  a  single  compact  statolith,  but  decapods  have 
two  additional  maculae  carrying  numerous  small  stato- 
conia.  Where  three  maculae  are  present,  they  are  set  in 
different  planes,  thus  being  able  to  resolve  linear  accel- 
erations in  any  direction. 


221 


222 


R.  WILLIAMSON 


Dorsal 


B 


Forward  view  Rear  view 


Ventral 


Figure  1.  (A)  Diagram  of  the  statocyst  of  Octopus  viewed  from  the 
side.  The  statocyst  sac  is  suspended  within  the  statocyst  cavity  by  fibrous 
strands.  There  are  two  areas  of  receptor  epithelium:  a  single,  oval  shaped 
macula  with  an  attached  statolith,  and  a  crista  strip  that  passes  around 
the  inside  of  the  sac.  such  that  it  covers  all  three  planes.  The  crista  strip 
is  divided  into  9  segments,  each  segment  carries  a  cupula  (not  shown). 
After  Budelmann.  1980.  (B)  A  forward  and  rear  view  of  the  cut  open 
statocyst  of  Sepia  officinalix.  There  are  3  maculae,  arranged  in  3  different 
planes,  and  the  crista  strip  is  divided  into  4  segments.  Anticristae  and 
hamuli  project  into  the  cavity  of  the  statocyst.  After  Budelmann.  1980. 


The  second  area  of  receptor  epithelium  consists  of  a 
narrow  strip  of  sensory  hair  cells  that  runs  around  the 
inside  of  the  statocyst  such  that  it  covers  all  three  planes 
(Fig.  1).  This  strip  is  usually  divided  into  segments:  the 
crista  segments,  each  carrying  a  cupula  attached  along  the 
length  of  the  crista  segment.  Octopods  (excluding  cirroc- 
topods)  have  nine  crista  segments,  whereas  decapods  have 
four,  each  with  its  own  cupula.  Rotational  movements  of 
the  animal  cause  a  flow  of  endolymph  relative  to  the  sta- 
tocyst wall;  this  flow  in  turn  deflects  the  cupula  and  stim- 
ulates the  underlying  hair  cells.  A  transverse  section 
through  a  crista  segment  (Fig.  2)  reveals  three  main  types 
of  cells  in  the  sensory  epithelium:  primary  sensory  hair 
cells,  secondary  sensory  hair  cells,  and  afferent  neurons. 
This  combination  of  primary  sensory  hair  cells  and  sec- 
ondary sensory  hair  cells  in  a  single  epithelium  is  unique 
to  cephalopods  (Budelmann  el  al..  1987).  Although  the 
crista/cupula  system  responds  principally  to  angular  ac- 
celerations, it  may  also  respond  to  linear  accelerations 
(Budelmann  and  Wolff,  1973;  Williamson  and  Budel- 
mann, 1 985a).  Because  the  crista/cupula  system  is  crucial 
for  signalling  most  of  the  animal's  movements,  and  be- 
cause this  system  is  dependent  upon  the  physical  param- 


eters of  the  statocyst,  we  will  concentrate  on  the  responses 
of  the  crista. 

Statocyst  size  and  shape 

The  idea  that  the  size  and  shape  of  the  statocyst  are 
correlated  with  its  likely  response  characteristics,  and 
hence  with  the  animal's  locomotory  performance,  arises 
from  the  physical  models  of  the  operation  of  the  vertebrate 
semicircular  canal  system  (Steinhausen,  1933;  Wilson  and 
Jones,  1979)  and  from  comparisons  of  canal  dimensions 
in  different  animals  (Jones  and  Spells,  1963;  Jones,  1984; 
Gauldie  and  Radtke.  1990)  and  in  animals  of  different 
sizes  (Curthoys,  1983).  The  Steinhausen  torsion  pendulum 
model  (Steinhausen,  1933;  Oman  et  al..  1987)  identifies 
the  radius  of  curvature  of  the  canal,  the  bore  radius  of 
the  canal  duct,  the  viscosity  and  density  of  the  endolymph. 
and  the  stiffness  of  the  cupula  as  being  important  factors 
determining  the  vestibular  response  characteristics.  Al- 
though, in  vertebrates,  there  is  a  good  correlation  between 
the  frequency  sensitivity  predicted  from  measurements 
of  the  radius  of  curvature  of  the  canal  and  the  bore  radius 
of  canal  duct,  and  the  actual  response  characteristics 
(Correia  et  al..  1981),  the  statocyst  position  is  much  less 
clear. 

The  use  of  such  a  model  in  cephalopods  is  supported 
by  the  relatively  large  size  of  the  statocysts  in  newly 
hatched  coleoids.  Those  statocysts  are  more  than  a  quarter 
of  the  mantle  length,  but  grow  at  a  much  lower  rate  than 
the  animal;  i.e..  they  increase  in  size  by  about  29  times 
while  the  mantle  length  is  increasing  by  390  times  (Mad- 
dock  and  Young,  1984).  This  relative  conservation  of 
statocyst  size  fits  well  with  the  idea  that  statocyst  size  is 
constrained  by  the  physical  principles  under  which  the 
organ  operates,  and  that  the  dimensions  of  the  system  are 
adjusted  to  the  speed  at  which  the  animal  turns.  In  ad- 
dition, Maddock  and  Young  (Maddock  and  Young,  1984; 
Young,  1984.  1989)  have  described  a  number  of  corre- 
lations between  statocyst  morphology  and  probable 
swimming  performance,  including  data  showing  that  the 
faster  moving  squids  tend  to  have  a  narrower  canal,  thus 


Ventral 


Figure  2.  Diagram  of  a  cross-section  through  the  crista  strip  of  the 
squid.  Alfali'iithis  xiibulala.  Three  main  cell  types  are  present:  the  primary 
sensory  hair  cells  (lightly  stippled),  the  secondary  sensory  hair  cells  (darkly 
stippled),  and  the  afferent  neurons  (unstippled).  After  Williamson,  I989a. 


STATOCYST  RESPONSE  CHARACTERISTICS 


223 


presumably  improving  the  high  frequency  response, 
whereas  slow  moving  cephalopods  tend  to  have  relatively 
large  statocysts,  thus  increasing  their  low  frequency  sen- 
sitivity. 

As  pointed  out  by  Young  ( 1984),  the  main  difficulties 
in  applying  this  idea  to  cephalopod  statocysts  is  that  the 
radius  of  curvature  can  only  be  approximated  as  the  cross- 
sectional  diameter  of  the  statocyst,  and  there  is  only  rarely 
a  canal-like  structure  in  the  statocyst  formed  by  the  an- 
ticrista  and  hamuli.  In  addition,  although  there  are  rec- 
ognizable patterns  of  anticristae  and  hamuli  in  different 
groups  of  cephalopods,  it  is  unclear  how  these  projections 
affect  the  flow  of  endolymph.  Clearly,  we  need  a  more 
realistic  model  of  how  the  endolymph  flows  within  the 
statocyst.  and  how  this  is  influenced  by  the  various  mor- 
phological features  of  the  statocyst. 

Cupula  parameters 

Other  morphological  features  likely  to  effect  the  fre- 
quency response  and  sensitivity  of  the  statocyst  angular 
acceleration  receptor  system  are  the  size,  shape,  and  at- 
tachment of  the  cupulae.  The  cupulae  are  gelatinous,  flap- 
like  structures,  projecting  towards  the  middle  of  the  stato- 
cyst, and  attached  to  the  crista  ridge  along  the  whole  length 
of  a  segment.  The  cupulae  however,  appear  to  be  irregular 
in  shape,  often  being  much  taller  in  the  center  of  the  crista 
segment  than  at  the  edges;  this  is  particularly  prominent 
in  the  squid,  Allolcul/iis  (Fig.  3a).  The  center  of  the  cupula 
will  therefore  present  a  much  greater  area  of  resistance  to 
endolymph  flow  than  the  edges  and  hence,  unless  the  cu- 
pula is  very  rigid,  will  more  easily  stimulate  the  underlying 
hair  cells.  This  likely  differential  sensitivity  in  different 
parts  of  a  single  crista  segment  may  be  a  method  of  frac- 
tionating the  sensitivity  range  of  the  system.  In  Octopus 
this  is  even  more  pronounced  (Fig.  3b.c).  Here,  the  nine 
crista  segments  have  alternating  large  and  small  cupulae. 
with  the  tall  cupulae  having  narrower  bases  than  the  small 
ones  (Budelmann  el  ai,  1987).  This,  again,  is  likely  to 
fractionate  the  range  over  which  the  system  operates  and, 
indeed,  recordings  from  the  afferent  neurons  in  represen- 
tatives of  these  two  different  segments  indicate  that  the 
segment  with  the  large  cupula  is  up  to  10  times  more 
sensitive  than  that  with  the  small  cupula  (Williamson  and 
Budelmann,  1985a,b).  The  increase  in  sensitivity  means, 
however,  that  the  afferents  from  the  large  segment  can  be 
driven  into  response  saturation  at  a  much  lower  stimulus 
intensity  than  those  from  the  small  cupula  segment.  This 
arrangement  could  be  correlated  with  Octopus '  two  forms 
of  locomotion,  the  high  sensitivity,  large  cupulae  being 
needed  during  slow  crawling  movements,  and  the  low 
sensitivity,  small  cupulae  during  jet  propelled  movements. 

Like  the  statoliths,  anticristae,  and  hamuli,  the  cupulae 
are  also  likely  to  have  an  effect  on  the  pattern  of  endo- 
lymph flow  within  the  statocyst.  Although  Young  (Mad- 


I 


100pm 


5O>um 


. 

I 


50 /urn 


Figure  3.  (A)  Crista  cupula  from  the  squid,  Alloteuthis  subulala. 
The  cupula  has  been  fixed  in  osmium  and  then  detached  from  the  crista 
segment.  Note  that  it  has  a  large  central  mass  and  is  much  shorter  at  the 
edges.  From  Williamson  I990a.  (B  and  C)  Transverse  sections  through 
two  different  crista  segments  in  the  statocyst  of  Octopus  showing  a  small, 
wide-based  cupula  type  and  a  large,  narrow-based  cupula  type.  From 
Williamson  and  Budelmann,  I985b. 

dock  and  Young,  1 984;  Young,  1 989)  has  used  a  verte- 
brate semicircular  canal  model  to  predict  endolymph  flow, 
and  hence  sensory  response  characteristics,  this  is  unlikely 
to  be  adequate.  Recent  vertebrate  models  have  shown  that 
even  a  good  canal  structure,  with  the  three  canals  or- 
thogonally arranged,  is  likely  to  have  a  complicated  pat- 
tern of  endolymph  flow  with  crosstalk  between  the  canals 
(Omanetal..  1987;  Mullerand  Verhagen,  1988).  In  ceph- 
alopods, which  rarely  have  a  single  canal  structure,  en- 
dolymph flow  patterns  are  extraordinarily  difficult  to  pre- 
dict (Govardovskii,  1971;  Muller,  pers.  comm.).  Even  the 
manner  of  movement  of  the  cupula  is  unknown;  i.e.. 
whether  it  pivots  like  a  lever,  or  slides  like  a  piston,  or 
flexes  like  a  diaphram,  although  recent  modelling  work 
has  suggested  that  the  cupula  does  not  operate  as  a  simple 
pivot  (Morris,  1 988). 

Another  unknown  with  respect  to  cupula  movement 
is  the  strength  of  its  attachment  to  the  crista  and  the  re- 
storing force  it  develops  when  displaced.  This  will  have  a 


224 


R.  WILLIAMSON 


major  impact  on  the  frequency  response  characteristics 
of  the  crista/cupula  system,  and  any  variation  between 
crista  segments,  or  between  different  animals,  would  have 
to  be  taken  into  account  in  a  model  describing  statocyst 
response  characteristics. 

The  presence  or  absence  of  a  perilymphatic  space  may 
also  affect  the  sensitivity  of  the  statocyst.  The  octopods, 
cirroctopods,  and  I  'ampyroteuthis  all  have  a  lymph-filled 
space  between  the  cartilaginous  wall  of  the  statocyst  cavity 
and  the  statocyst  sac  containing  the  sensory  epithelia.  An- 
liker  and  van  Buskirk  (1971),  dealing  with  the  vertebrate 
semicircular  canal  system,  have  argued  that  the  movement 
of  perilymph  may  have  a  major  effect  on  the  dynamic 
response  characteristics  of  the  system.  Although  any  peri- 
lymph  flow  in  the  statocyst  would  be  restricted  by  the 
fibers  supporting  the  statocyst  sac,  there  may  well  be  an 
effect  in  cephalopods  from  this  source. 

Physiological  Features 

Extracellular  recordings  from  statocyst  afferents  have 
shown  that  the  crista/cupula  system  in  Octopus  acts  as  a 
velocity  transducer  over  a  middle  range  of  frequencies 
and  has  response  characteristics  similar  to  those  of  the 
vertebrate  semicircular  canal  system  (Williamson  and 
Budelmann,  1985a).  There  is  as  yet  no  data  on  afferent 
response  characteristics  from  decapod  statocysts.  Recent 
intracellular  recordings  from  hair  cells  in  the  statocyst  of 
the  squid.  Alloteut/iis  subulata.  have  provided  the  first 
measurements  of  the  sensitivities  of  cephalopod  hair  cells 
(Williamson.  199 la).  This  work  (Fig.  4)  has  shown  that 
the  secondary  sensory  hair  cells  in  the  crista  have  sensi- 
tivities of  at  least  0.5  mV  per  degree  of  cilia  deflection. 
This  compares  with  sensitivities  of  about  3  m  V  per  degree 
for  frog  saccular  hair  cells  (Hudspeth  and  Corey,  1977), 
10  mV  per  degree  for  turtle  basilar  papillar  hair  cells 
(Crawford  and  Fettiplace,  1985),  and  30  mV  per  degree 
for  mouse  cochlear  hair  cells  (Russell  et  ai,  1986).  This 
work  has  also  confirmed  morphological  studies  (Budel- 
mann el  a/.,  1987)  showing  that  at  least  some  of  the  sec- 
ondary hair  cells  are  physiologically  polarized  in  the  op- 
posite direction  to  the  primary  hair  cells  (Fig.  4).  This 
bipolar  sensitivity  does  not  occur  in  vertebrate  vestibular 
cristae  and,  although  it  may  be  more  energy  efficient 
(Williamson,  199 la),  it  is  not  clear  if  it  will  have  any 
effect  on  the  sensitivity  or  frequency  bandwidth  of  the 
system. 

Differences  in  hair  cell  sensitivity 

There  may  well  be  differences  in  the  intrinsic  sensitiv- 
ities of  the  individual  crista  hair  cells.  In  Octopus,  there 
are  at  least  three  different  morphological  types  of  crista 
hair  cells:  the  primary  sensory  hair  cells,  the  small  sec- 
ondary sensory  hair  cells,  and  the  large  secondary  sensory 
hair  cells.  In  addition,  there  are  different  types  of  afferent 


_J 

50ms 


5mV 


3Mm 


B 


\ 


1mV 


6|.im 


Figure  4.  Intracellular  recordings  from  a  primary  sensory  hair  cell 
(A)  and  a  secondary  sensory  hair  cell  (B)  in  the  crista  of  the  squid  statocyst. 
showing  their  responses  to  small  mechanical  displacements  of  the  over- 
lying cupula  (displacements  shown  in  lower  traces).  Note  that  the  primary 
and  secondary  hair  cell  depolarizations  are  caused  by  cupula  displace- 
ments of  opposite  directions,  indicating  that  the  cells  are  polarized  in 
opposing  directions,  and  that  only  the  primary  hair  cell  carries  action 
potentials.  From  Williamson,  1 99 la. 


neurons,  and  there  may  also  be  subdivisions  of  the  hair 
cells  types  (Budelmann  et  a/.,  1987).  These  morphological 
differences  are  likely  to  be  reflected  in  physiological  cell 
parameters,  such  as  input  impedance  and  cell  conduc- 
tance, and  therefore  result  in  differences  in  the  sensitivities 
of  the  various  cell  types  (Williamson  and  Budelmann, 
1985a). 

Electrical  coupling 

At  least  some  of  the  secondary  sensory  hair  cells  in  the 
squid  statocyst  cristae  are  known  to  be  electrically  coupled 
along  the  length  of  the  crista  segment  (Fig.  5)  (Williamson, 
1989a).  It  has  been  argued  that  this  coupling  will  lead  to 
an  improvement  in  the  signal  to  noise  ratio  of  the  system 
and  hence  enhance  its  overall  sensitivity.  However,  such 
coupling  is  also  likely  to  lower  the  high  frequency  response 
of  the  system.  Clearly,  if  the  coupling  could  be  varied 
under  direct  nervous  control,  this  would  be  a  powerful 
mechanism  for  changing  the  sensitivity  and  frequency  re- 
sponse of  the  system.  A  comparable  sensory  system  with 


STATOCYST  RESPONSE  CHARACTERISTICS 


Electrotonic  coupling 


CelM 


Cell  2 


5mV 


Current 


50ms 


1_ 


|4nA 


Figure  5.  Intracellular  recordings  from  two  nearby  secondary  sensory 
hair  cells  in  the  statocyst  crista  of  the  cuttlefish.  Sepia  officinalis,  showing 
their  electrotonic  coupling.  A  small  current  (bottom  trace)  is  injected 
into  Cell  1  (top  tracel,  producing  a  depolarization,  and  this  causes  a 
simultaneous,  but  smaller,  depolarization  in  the  neighboring  cells  (Cell 
2,  middle  trace).  This  provides  evidence  that  the  secondary  sensory  hair 
cells  in  a  crista  segment  are  electrotonically  coupled  along  the  segment. 
From  Williamson,  1991K 


neurally  controlled  electrical  coupling  is  in  the  vertebrate 
retina,  where  the  neurotransmitter  dopamine  alters  the 
coupling  ratio  between  retinal  horizontal  cells  (Knapp  and 
Dowling,  1987).  Dopamine  has  been  located  in  the  retinal 
efferents  in  Octopus  (Suzuki  and  Tasaki,  1983)  and  has 
also  been  tentatively  identified  in  the  statocyst  efferents 
(Budelmann  and  Bonn,  1982;  Williamson,  1989b).  It 
would  be  an  astonishing  example  of  parallel  evolution  if 
these  two  disparate  sense  organs,  the  eye  and  the  statocyst, 
used  dopaminergic  control  of  electrical  coupling  to  reg- 
ulate their  sensory  input. 

Efferent  system 

The  statocysts  have  an  exceptionally  large  efferent  in- 
nervation;  of  the  axons  in  the  Octopus  statocyst  crista 
nerves,  75%  are  efferent  fibers  travelling  from  the  brain 
to  the  statocyst  (Budelmann  el  ai,  1987).  In  contrast, 
about  8%  of  axons  in  a  vertebrate  vestibular  nerve  are 
efferents  (Goldberg  and  Fernandez,  1980).  This  efferent 
innervation  forms  a  plexus  running  beneath  the  crista 
epithelium  and  makes  synaptic  contact  with  primary  and 
secondary  sensory  hair  cells,  as  well  as  with  the  afferent 
and  other  efferent  neurons  (Budelmann  et  ai,  1987).  The 
efferent  fibers  are  active  during  movements  of  the  animal's 
head  (Williamson,  1986)  and  can  depress  or  enhance  (Fig. 
6)  the  afferent  output  from  the  statocyst  (Williamson, 
1985).  These  effects  are  due  to  direct  synaptic  hyperpo- 
larization,  or  to  depolarization,  of  the  secondary  sensory 
hair  cells,  their  first-order  afferent  neurons,  and  possibly, 
the  primary  sensory  hair  cells  (Williamson,  1989c).  The 
inhibitory  response  is  probably  due  to  cholinergic  synapses 
(Auerbach  and  Budelmann,  1986;  Williamson,  1989b), 
and  the  excitatory  response  to  catecholaminergic  synapses 
(Budelmann  and  Bonn,  1982;  Williamson,  1989b). 


Such  a  widespread  and  complex  efferent  innervation 
provides  the  animal  with  direct  and  independent  control 
of  both  the  hair  cell  receptor  potential  and  the  level  of 
activity  of  the  afferent  neurons.  Thus,  not  only  can  the 
gain  of  the  overall  system  be  increased  or  decreased,  but 
the  responses  of  individual  elements  can  also  be  varied. 
This  permits  an  extension  of  the  dynamic  range  of  the 
system  by  allowing  adjustments  to  the  membrane  poten- 
tials of  the  hair  cells  and  afferent  neurons,  so  that  the 
cells'  responses  are  maintained  within  their  operating 
ranges  and  at  their  maximum  sensitivities. 

Motile  cilia  and  cells 

Another  feature  that  may  have  an  impact  on  the  sen- 
sitivity of  the  statocyst  hair  cells  is  the  presence  of  motile 
cilia.  Ciliated  cells  are  distributed  all  over  the  inner  surface 
of  the  statocyst.  as  well  as  in  Kolliker's  canal  (Young, 
1960);  these  cells  have  beating  cilia  that  set  up  minute 
endolymph  currents  within  the  statocyst  (Budelmann, 
1990).  The  biological  significance  of  these  cells  is  not  clear, 
but  the  fluid  flow  that  they  produce  may  be  sufficient  to 
increase  the  background  noise  within  the  system  and  thus 
reduce  the  overall  sensitivity  of  the  receptor  system. 

In  addition  to  these  ciliated  cells,  which  are  motile, 
some  of  the  sensory  hair  cells  within  the  crista  or  macula 
epithelia  may  also  have  a  motor  capability.  Sensory  cells 
with  motile  beating  cilia  are  present  in  the  statocysts  of 
other  mollusks  (Stommel  et  ai.  1980),  and  some  circum- 


60 


30 


m 
>-    o 

0) 

a 

V) 
Q) 

I60 


30 


16 

Time  (sec) 


32 


Figure  6.  Peristimulus  time  histograms  showing  the  effect  of  efferent 
activity  on  the  statocyst  afferent  activity.  Extracellular  recordings  were 
obtained  from  afferent  neurons  from  the  Octopus  crista  and  then  efferents 
to  this  segment  activated  by  electrical  stimulation  (duration  and  time 
indicated  by  heavy  bar  on  time  axes).  This  caused  an  inhibition  of  the 
activity  of  unit  A,  but  an  increase  in  the  activity  of  unit  B.  This  provides 
evidence  that  there  are  both  inhibitory  and  excitatory  efferents  innervating 
the  statocyst  crista.  Bin  width,  400  ms;  stimulus,  50  Hz  pulses  for  6  s. 
From  Williamson.  1985. 


226 


R.  WILLIAMSON 


stantial  evidence  suggests  that  part  of  the  membrane  po- 
tential noise  in  recordings  from  some  secondary  sensory 
hair  cells  in  squid  crista  may  be  due  to  ciliary  movement 
(Williamson,  1991a).  If  some  of  the  hair  cells  in  the  cnsta 
or  macula  do  have  a  motor  capability,  this  could  have  a 
large  impact  on  the  responses  of  the  system.  Recent  work 
on  vertebrate  hair  cells  has  shown  that  motility  in  the 
outer  hair  cells  of  the  cochlea  can  change  the  response 
characteristics  of  the  sensory  system  by  altering  the  mi- 
cromechanics  of  the  basilar  membrane  responses  (Hud- 
speth,  1989).  This  is  thought  to  be  due  to  changes  in  the 
length  of  the  cells  rather  than  an  active  beating  of  their 
cilia.  Such  a  system,  operating  under  efferent  control, 
could  also  be  present  in  the  cephalopod  statocyst. 

Central  processing 

A  final  feature  that  can  influence  the  characteristics  of 
the  statocyst  input  is  the  central  processing  of  the  statocyst 
information.  This  has  two  major  functions:  first,  the  cen- 
tral control  of  the  statocyst  efferents,  and  second,  the  an- 
alytical processing  of  the  statocyst  afferent  information. 
As  has  already  been  discussed,  the  efferents  can  have  a 
major  impact  on  the  response  characteristics  of  the  stato- 
cyst. This  can  operate  through  a  variety  of  mechanisms. 
In  a  feed-forward  system,  for  example,  where  the  animal 
makes  a  voluntary  movement  such  as  a  jet  propelled  es- 
cape, the  efferents  can  be  used  to  suppress,  peripherally, 
the  massive  input  from  the  statocysts  that  may  saturate 
the  afferent  system.  This  could  also  be  achieved  centrally 
by  an  efference  copy  mechanism,  as  has  been  proposed 
for  fish  electroreception  (Bell,  1981).  Additionally,  the 
system  may  operate  in  a  feedback  mode,  whereby  the 
afferent  input  feeds  back  through  the  efferents  to  dynam- 
ically adjust  the  sensitivity  of  the  system  (Williamson, 
1986).  This  may  be  important  in  sustained  swimming  or 
in  movements  imposed  by  external  water  currents.  Where 
the  efferents  are  acting  at  the  periphery,  the  frequency 
response  of  the  system  may  well  be  limited  by  the  con- 
duction velocities  of  the  efferents.  The  efferent  axons  are 
small,  unmyelinated  fibers  (Budelmann  el  al,  1987)  and 
are  likely  to  have  much  slower  conduction  velocities  than 
the  larger  afferent  fibers. 

The  statocyst  afferents  project  to  the  ipsi-  and  contra- 
lateral  lateral  pedal,  pedal,  and  ventral  magnocellular  lobes 
within  the  suboesophageal  mass  of  the  octopus  brain  (Bu- 
delmann and  Young,  1984;  Plan,  1987).  Probably,  the 
sensitivity  of  the  sensory  system  can  be  improved  centrally 
by  summing  multiple  afferent  inputs.  For  example,  where 
ipsi-  and  contralateral  statocyst  inputs  are  from  receptors 
responding  to  the  same  direction  of  movement,  then  these 
multiple  channel  inputs  could  be  combined  to  improve 
the  sensitivity  of  the  system  or  to  reduce  the  noise  in  the 
system  (Aidley,  1971).  This  could  also  occur  at  the  pe- 
riphery, where  some  afferent  neurons,  in  both  crista  and 


macula,  receive  multiple  inputs  from  a  number  of  nearby 
hair  cells  (Colmers,  1981;  Budelmann  el  al.  1987). 

Future  research 

The  idea  of  being  able  to  predict  the  locomotory  per- 
formance of  a  cephalopod  solely  from  the  morphology  of 
its  statocysts  is  very  attractive,  especially  because  all  but 
a  few  species  are  unavailable  for  free  swimming  studies 
or  for  physiological  testing.  However,  although  such  pre- 
diction based  on  a  study  of  the  vestibular  system  is  now 
feasible  for  vertebrates,  only  generalized  statements  can 
be  made  about  cephalopods. 

There  are  two  main  reasons  for  this.  First,  there  is  no 
hydrodynamic  model  of  endolymph  flow  within  the 
statocyst  that  takes  into  account  the  special  features  of 
the  statocysts.  Although  vertebrate  semicircular  canal 
models  are  a  good  starting  point,  we  can  have  only  limited 
confidence  in  the  accuracy  of  predictions  transported  di- 
rectly into  the  cephalopod  domain.  Second,  there  is  no 
base  of  physiological  work  on  cephalopods  to  provide  the 
constants  needed  fora  mathematical  description  of  stato- 
cyst performance,  or  to  test  and  refine  any  model  predic- 
tions. For  example,  even  the  best  model  based  on  mor- 
phological studies,  could  not  predict  the  effects  of  electrical 
coupling  or  motile  cilia  on  the  afferent  response  charac- 
teristics. 

Future  work,  therefore,  should  be  concentrated  on  de- 
veloping an  adequate  model  of  statocyst  endolymph  flow, 
including  a  description  of  cupula  movement.  This  should 
be  complemented  by  an  investigation  of  the  afferent  re- 
sponse characteristics  of  representatives  of  the  different 
cephalopod  groups.  These  data,  together  with  a  description 
of  the  swimming  styles  and  the  likely  accelerations  pro- 
duced in  a  few  species  of  cephalopods.  should  give  us 
sufficient  information  to  predict  with  some  confidence 
the  probable  locomotory  performance  of  an  animal,  based 
only  on  the  morphology  of  its  statocyst. 

Acknowledgments 

1  would  like  to  thank  Prof.  J.  Z.  Young  for  his  unending 
stimulation,  enthusiasm,  and  encouragement.  Much  of 
this  work  was  supported  by  the  Alexander  von  Humboldt 
Stiftung  and  the  Wellcome  Trust. 

Literature  Cited 

Aidley,  D.  J.  1971 .  The  Physiology  of  Excitable  Cells  Cambridge  Uni- 
versity Press,  London.  Pp.  306-333. 

Anliker,  M.,  and  \V.  van  Buskirk.  1971.  The  role  of  perilymph  in  the 
responses  of  the  semicircular  canals  to  angular  accelerations.  Ada 
Ololaryngol.  72:93-100. 

Auerbach,  B.,  and  B.  U.  Budelmann.  1986.  Evidence  for  acetylcholine 
as  a  neurotransmitter  in  the  statocyst  of  Octopus  vulgaris.  Cell  Tissue 
Res  243:  429-436. 

Bell,  C.  C.  1981.  An  efference  copy  which  is  modified  by  reafference 
input.  Science  214:  450-453. 


STATOCYST  RESPONSE  CHARACTERISTICS 


227 


Budelmann,  B.  I'.  1980.  Equilibrium  and  orientation  in  cephalopods. 
Oceania  23:  34-43. 

Budelmann,  B.  V.  1990.  The  statocysts  of  squid.  In  Squid  ax  Experi- 
mental Animals.  D.  L.  Gilbert.  W.  J.  Adelman  and  J.  M.  Arnold, 
eds.  Plenum  Press.  New  York. 

Itiuli  Iniaiin.  B.  t '.,  and  I1.  Bonn.  1982.  Histochemical  evidence  for 
catecholamines  as  neurotransmitters  in  the  statocyst  of  Octopus  vul- 
garis.  Cell  Tissue  Res  227:  475-483. 

Budelmann,  B.  U.,  M.  Sachse,  and  M.  Staudigl.  1987.  The  angular 
acceleration  receptor  system  of  the  statocyst  of  Octopus  vu/garis: 
morphometry.  ultrastructure,  and  neuronal  and  synaptic  organization. 
Phil  Trans.  R  Soc.  Loud.  B  315:  305-343. 

Budelmann,  B.  II.,  and  H.  G.  Wolff.  1973.  Gravity  response  from  an- 
gular acceleration  receptors  in  Octopus  vulgaris.  J.  Comp.  Physio!. 
85:  283-290. 

Budelmann,  B.  I'.,  and  J.  Z.  Young.  1984.  The  statocyst-oculomotor 
system  of  Octopus  vulgaris:  extraocular  eye  muscles,  eye  muscle 
nerves,  statocyst  nerves  and  the  oculomotor  centre  in  the  central 
nervous  system.  Phil.  Trans.  R  Soc.  Land.  B  306:  159-189. 

Colmers,  \V.  F.  1981.  Afferent  synaptic  connections  between  hair  cells 
and  the  somata  of  intramacular  neurons  in  the  gravity  receptor  system 
of  the  statocyst  of  Octopus  vu/garis.  J  Comp  Neurol.  197:  385-394. 

Correia,  M.  J..  J.  P.  Landolt,  M.-D.  Ni.  A.  R.  Eden,  and  J.  L.  Rae. 
1981.  A  species  comparison  of  linear  and  nonlinear  transfer  char- 
acteristics of  primary  afferents  innervating  the  semicircular  canal.  In 
The  Veslibular  System:  Function  and  Morphology,  T.  Gualtierotti. 
ed.  Springer  Verlag,  New  York. 

Crawford,  A.  C.,  and  R.  Feltiplace.  1985.  The  mechanical  properties 
of  ciliary  bundles  of  turtle  cochlear  hair  cells.  /  Phyxiol.  364:  359- 
380. 

Curthoys,  I.S.  1983.  The  development  of  function  of  primary  vestibular 
neurons.  Pp.  425-461  In  Development  ol  Auditory  and  Vestibular 
Systems.  E.  Romand,  ed.  Academic  Press,  New  York. 

Gauldie,  R.  W.,  and  R.  L.  Radtke.  1990.  Using  the  physical  dimensions 
of  the  semicircular  canal  as  a  probe  to  evaluate  inner  ear  function 
in  fishes.  Comp.  Biochem.  Physiol.  96A:  199-203. 

Goldberg,  J.  M.,  and  C.  Fernandez.  1980.  Efferent  vestibular  system 
in  the  squirrel  monkey:  anatomical  location  and  influence  on  afferent 
activity.  J.  \europhysiol.  43:  986-1025. 

Govardovskii,  V.  I.  1971.  Some  characteristics  and  dynamics  of  the 
endolymph  in  cephalopod  statocysts.  J.  Evol.  Biochem  Physiol.  1: 
347-351. 

Hudspeth,  A.J.  1989.     How  the  ear's  ear  works.  Science  341:  397-404. 

Hudspeth,  A.  J.,  and  D.  P.  Corey.  1977.  Sensitivity,  polarity  and  con- 
ductance change  in  the  response  of  vertebrate  hair  cells  to  controlled 
mechanical  stimuli.  Proc.  Nat/.  Acad.  Sci.  USA.  74:  2407-241 1. 

Jones,  G.  M.  1984.  The  functional  significance  of  semicircular  canal 
size.  In  Handbook  of  Sensory  Physiology.  Vol.  6  (part  1),  H.  H. 
Kornhuber,  ed.  Springer- Verlag,  New  York. 

Jones,  G.  M.,  and  K.  E.  Spells.  1963.  A  theoretical  and  comparative 
study  of  the  functional  dependence  of  the  semicircular  canal  upon 
its  physical  dimensions.  Proc.  R  Soc.  Land.  B.  157:  403-419. 

knapp,  A.G.,  and  J.  E.  Dowling.  1987.  Dopamine  enhances  excitatory 
amino  acid-gated  conductances  in  cultured  retinal  horizontal  cells. 
Nature  325:  437-439. 

Maddock.  L.,  and  J.  7..  Y'oung.  1984.  Some  dimensions  of  the  angular 
acceleration  receptor  systems  of  cephalopods.  J.  Afar.  Biol.  Assoc. 
U.K.  64:  55-79. 


Morris,  C.  C.  1988.     Statolith  growth  lines  and  statocyst  function  in 

the  cephalopoda.  Ph.D.  thesis.  University  of  Cambridge.  U.K. 
Muller,  M.,  and  J.  H.  G.  Verhagen.  1988.     A  new  quantitative  model 

of  total  endolymph  flow  in  the  system  of  semicircular  ducts. ./  Theor 

Bio/.  134:473-501. 
Oman,  C.  M.,  E.  N.  Marcus,  and  1.  S.  Curthoys.  1987.     The  influence 

of  semicircular  canal  morphology  on  endolymph  flow  dynamics.  Acta 

Otolaryngol.  (Slockli.)  103:  1-13. 
Plan,  T.  1987.     Functional  neuroanatomy  of  the  sensory-motor  lobes 

of  the  brain  of  Octopus  vulgaris.  Ph.D.  thesis.  University  of  Regens- 

burg.  West  Germany. 
Russell,  I.  J.,  A.  R.  Cody,  and  G.  P.  Richardson.  1986.     The  responses 

of  inner  and  outer  hair  cells  in  the  basal  turn  of  the  guinea-pig  cochlea 

and  in  the  mouse  cochlea  grown  in  vitro.  Hearing  Res.  22:  199-2 16. 
Steinhausen,  W.  1933.     Uber  die  Beobachtung  der  Cupula  in  den  Bo- 

gengangsampullen  des  Labyrinths  des  lebenden  Hechts.  P/liigers  Arch. 

Ges.  Physio/.  232:  500-512. 
Stephens,  P.  R.,  and  J.  Z.  Young.  1982.     The  statocysts  of  the  squid 

Loligo.  J  Zoo/.  Loud.  197:  241-266. 

Stommel,  E.  W.,  R.  E.  Stephens,  and  D.  L.  Alkon.  1980.     Motile  stato- 
cyst cilia  transmit  rather  than  directly  transduce  mechanical  stimuli. 

/  Cell  Bio/  87:  652-662. 
Suzuki,  H.,  and  K.  Tasaki.  1983.     Inhibitory  retinal  efferents  from  do- 

paminergic  cells  in  the  optic  lobe  of  the  Octopus.  I  'ision  Res.  23: 

451-457. 
Williamson,  R.  1985.     Efferent  influences  on  the  afferent  activity  from 

the  octopus  angular  acceleration  receptor  system.  /  E.\p.  Biol  1 19: 

251-254. 
Williamson,  R.  1986.     Efferent  activity  in  the  Octopus  statocyst  nerves. 

/  Comp.  Physiol  A.  158:  125-132. 
Williamson,  R.  1989a.     Electrical  coupling  between  secondary  hair  cells 

in  the  statocyst  of  the  squid.  Alloteiitliis  subulata  Brain  Res  486: 

67-72. 
Williamson,  R.  I989b.     Electrophysiological  evidence  for  cholinergic 

and  catecholaminergic  efferent  transmitters  in  the  statocyst  of  Oc- 
topus. Comp.  Biochem.  Physiol.  93C:  23-27. 
Williamson.  R.  1989c.     Secondary  hair  cells  and  afferent  neurones  of 

the  squid  statocyst  receive  both  inhibitory  and  excitatory  efferent 

inputs.  J.  Comp.  Physiol.  A.  165:  847-860. 
Williamson,  R.  1991a.     The  responses  of  primary  and  secondary  sensory 

hair  cells  in  the  squid  statocyst  to  mechanical  stimulation.  J.  Comp. 

Physio/.  A.  167:  655-664. 
Williamson,  R.  1991b.     The  responses  of  the  sensory  hair  cells  in  the 

statocyst  of  Sepia.  In  The  Cuttlefish,  E.  Boucaud-Camou,  ed.  Uni- 
versity de  Caen  Press,  Caen,  France. 
Williamson,  R.,  and  B.  U.  Budelmann.  1985a.     The  responses  of  the 

Octopus  angular  acceleration  receptor  system  to  sinusoidal  stimu- 
lation. /  Comp.  Physiol.  A.  156:  403-412. 
Williamson,  R.,  and  B.  U.  Budelmann.  I985b.     An  angular  acceleration 

receptor  system  of  dual  sensitivity  in  the  statocyst  of  Octopus  vulgaris. 

Experientia  41:  1321-1323. 

Wilson,  V.  J.,  and  G.  M.  Jones.  1979.     Mammalian  \'eslibular  Phys- 
iology. Plenum  Press,  New  York. 
Y'oung,  J.  Z.  1960.     The  statocyst  of  Octopus  vulgaris.  Proc.  R   Soc 

Land.  B  152:  3-29. 
Y'oung,  J.  Z.  1984.     The  statocysts  of  cranchiid  squids  (Cephalopoda). 

J.  Zool.  Land.  203:  1-21. 
Y'oung,  J.  Z.  1989.     The  angular  acceleration  receptor  system  of  diverse 

cephalopods.  Phil.  Trans.  R  Soc.  Land.  325:  1 89-238. 


Reference:  Biol.  Bull  180:  228-233.  (April.  1991) 


Neural  Control  of  Speed  Changes  in  an 
Opisthobranch  Locomotory  System 


RICHARD  A.  SATTERLIE 

Department  of  Zoology,  Arizona  Stale  University,  Tempe,  Arizona  85287-1501 
and  Friday  Harbor  Laboratories,  Friday  Harbor,  Washington  98250 


Abstract.  Three  forms  of  forward  locomotion  have  been 
described  in  the  pteropod  mollusk  C/ione  limaeina,  in- 
cluding slow,  fast,  and  escape  swimming.  The  neuro- 
muscular  organization  of  the  swimming  system  suggests 
that  a  two-geared  system  operates  for  slow  and  fast  swim- 
ming, while  the  escape  response  is  superimposed  on  fast 
swimming.  In  addition  to  escape,  changes  in  locomotory 
speed  can  occur  through  a  dramatic  "change-of-gears," 
or  through  a  more  subtle  change  of  speed  within  gears. 
The  former  involves  reconfiguration  of  the  central  pattern 
generator  and  recruitment  of  previously  inactive  motor 
units.  The  latter  can  be  due  to:  changes  in  tonic  inputs  to 
the  central  neurons,  central  modulation  that  is  not  suf- 
ficient to  "change  gears,"  endogenous  properties  of  muscle 
cells,  and  peripheral  modulation  of  muscle  contractility. 
The  initial  ballistic  phase  of  escape  swimming  is  believed 
to  be  triggered  by  activity  in  a  newly  identified  pair  of 
swim  motor  neurons  that  neither  receive  information 
from,  nor  provide  input  to,  the  central  pattern  generator. 
These  neurons  appear  to  produce  a  startle  response.  Ev- 
idence presented  suggests  that  most,  if  not  all,  of  these 
variables  help  produce  locomotory  plasticity  in  Clione. 

Introduction 

Locomotory  speed  is  a  function  of  several  factors,  most 
notably  the  frequency  of  movements  of  locomotory  ap- 
pendages and  the  force  of  appendage  movements.  A 
change  in  either  of  these  factors  can  directly  trigger  a 
change  in  locomotory  speed.  The  former  is  the  provence 
of  the  central  pattern  generator  circuitry,  whereas  the  latter 
can  be  linked  to  modifications  of  the  neuromuscular  sys- 
tem, and  can  conceivably  include  purely  peripheral  plas- 
ticity. Furthermore,  activity  of  central  and  peripheral 

Received  17  August  1990;  accepted  23  January  1991. 


modulators  can  serve  to  increase  the  richness  of  loco- 
motory variability. 

Few  preparations  are  conducive  to  simultaneous  elec- 
trophysiological  monitoring  of  both  central  and  peripheral 
activity  during  both  dramatic  and  subtle  changes  in  pro- 
pulsive activity.  One  preparation  that  combines  similar 
behavioral  variability  with  the  typical  advantages  of  the 
molluscan  nervous  system — relatively  simple  neural  or- 
ganization coupled  with  large  cell  size — is  the  locomotory 
system  of  the  pteropod  mollusk  Clione  limacina.  Thus 
far,  the  majority  of  work  on  Clione  has  centered  on  the 
central  generation  of  rhythmic  locomotory  activity  (Ar- 
shavsky  et  al,  1985a.  b.  c.  d,  1986,  1989;  Satterlie.  1985, 
1989;  Satterlie  and  Spencer,  1985;  Satterlie  et  al..  1985), 
although  recent  work  has  focussed  on  peripheral  neuro- 
muscular physiology  (Satterlie,  1987,  1988;  Satterlie  et 
al.,  1990).  The  purpose  of  this  review  is  to  summarize 
current  work  and  present  new  data  that  relate  to  the  neu- 
robiological  basis  of  locomotory  plasticity  in  the  Clione 
swimming  system. 

Results  and  Discussion 

Locomotory  movements  of  Clione  include  relatively 
simple  two-phase  flapping  movements  of  wing-like  para- 
podia  (wings).  Three  forms  of  locomotion  have  been  de- 
scribed including  slow,  fast,  and  escape  swimming  (Ar- 
shavskyt'M/.,  1985a;  Satterlie  et  al..  1985,  1990;  Satterlie, 
1989).  The  predominant  form  is  slow  swimming,  which 
allows  the  animal  to  maintain  position  in  the  water  col- 
umn or  to  move  forward  (upward)  slowly.  Wing  beat  fre- 
quencies observed  during  slow  swimming  ranged  from  1 
to  4  Hz.  Changes  in  the  rate  of  forward  movement  within 
the  slow  speed  occur  both  with  and  without  a  change  in 
the  frequency  of  wing  movements.  The  latter  cases  pre- 
sumably involve  changes  in  wing  contractility,  as  sug- 


228 


LOCOMOTORV  SPEED  CHANGES 


229 


gested  by  behavioral  observations  in  which  noticeable 
changes  in  the  vigor  of  wing  movements  have  been  ob- 
served in  the  absence  of  a  change  in  wing  beat  frequency. 
The  change  to  fast  swimming  is  a  triggered,  typically  dra- 
matic change  in  the  frequency  (range:  3-8  Hz)  and  force 
of  wing  movements.  In  addition  to  these  two  basic  forms 
of  swimming,  a  ballistic  escape  response  can  be  triggered 
following  vigorous  stimulation  of  the  tail  (Satterlie  et  ai. 
1990).  The  initial  phase  of  escape  swimming  involves  one 
or  two  wing  cycles  characterized  by  massive  contractions 
of  the  swim  musculature.  This  "startle"  phase  is  followed 
by  a  variable  period  of  enhanced  fast  swimming.  While 
fast  swimming  can  be  triggered  without  an  escape  response 
being  activated,  escape  is  always  followed  by  fast  swim- 
ming. 

Despite  the  three-phase  swimming  behavior,  both  the 
central  and  peripheral  organization  of  the  swimming  sys- 
tem appears  to  be  based  on  two  speeds.  Evidence  presented 
later  suggests  that  escape  swimming  is  merely  superim- 
posed on  the  fast  swimming  system.  Centrally,  the  change 
from  slow  to  fast  swimming  involves  a  "change-of-gears." 
defined  here  as  a  change  in  pattern  generator  output  that 
results  in  recruitment  (or  dropping  out)  of  motor  units 
that  have  significantly  different  biochemical  and  contrac- 
tile properties  than  those  that  were  previously  (or  contin- 
uously) active.  Peripherally,  Clione  has  two  types  of 
striated  swim  muscle  fibers:  slow-twitch  fatigue-resistant 
and  fast-twitch  fatigable  fibers  (Satterlie.  1987;  Satterlie 
et  ai.  1990).  To  complement  the  peripheral  organization, 
two  types  of  swim  motor  neurons  have  been  described: 
one  associated  with  slow-twitch  muscle  activity  (and  slow 
swimming),  and  the  other  associated  with  both  types  of 
muscle  fibers.  The  latter  motor  units,  which  include  two 
large  swim  motor  neurons  in  each  pedal  ganglion,  are 
recruited  into  activity  during  fast  swimming  (Satterlie. 
1987,  1988,  1989). 

With  the  two-geared  arrangement  of  the  Clione  swim- 
ming system  before  us,  three  categories  of  locomotory 
speed  changes  will  be  described,  with  evidence  presented 
to  suggest  neurobiological  mechanisms  for  each.  Cate- 
gories of  speed  change  mechanisms  include:  ( 1 )  change- 
of-gears,  (2)  change  of  speed  within  gears,  and  (3)  escape 
swimming. 


c;  Satterlie,  1985.  1989).  One  group  of  interneurons  (V- 
phase  interneurons)  produces  a  single  action  potential 
during  ventral  bending  of  the  wings,  whereas  the  other 
group  (D-phase  interneurons)  spikes  during  dorsal  bend- 
ing of  the  wings.  Alternating  activity  of  these  two  groups 
of  interneurons  continues  during  fast  swimming,  but  two 
additional  interneuron  types  become  active  (Arshavsky 
et  nl..  1985d,  1989).  Delayed  V-phase  interneurons,  which 
receive  only  inhibitory  input  from  D-phase  interneurons 
during  slow  swimming,  produce  slightly  delayed  (with  re- 
spect to  normal  V-phase  interneurons)  V-phase  spikes 
during  fast  swimming.  Spikes  in  the  delayed  V-phase  in- 
terneurons trigger  activity  in  the  second  type  of  interneu- 
ron, called  interneurons  12  (Arshavsky  et  ai.  1985d. 
1989).  Each  interneuron  12  produces  a  plateau  potential 
that  is  turned  on  by  excitatory  input  from  delayed  V- 
phase  interneurons,  and  is  turned  off  by  inhibitory  input 
from  D-phase  interneurons.  Plateau  potentials  of  inter- 
neurons 12  inhibit  V-phase  interneurons  and  excite  D- 
phase  interneurons.  Addition  of  the  delayed  V-phase  and 
type  12  interneurons  to  the  swim  pattern  generator  thus 
produces  an  early  termination  of  V-phase  activity  coupled 
with  onset  of  the  next  D-phase.  This  change  increases  the 
cycle  frequency  of  pattern  generator  output  (Arshavsky 
t'/  ai.  1985a)  and  is  associated  with  a  recruitment  of  pre- 
viously inactive  large  motor  neurons  (Fig.  1).  As  men- 
tioned previously,  recruitment  of  these  motor  neurons  is 
associated  with  the  activation  of  the  fast-twitch  muscu- 
lature of  the  wings.  The  change-of-gears  is  also  associated 
with  a  5- 1 5  m  V  tonic  depolarization  in  "normal"  D-  and 
V-phase  interneurons  of  the  swim  pattern  generator  (Fig. 
1).  The  combination  of  increased  cycle  frequency  and 
increased  force  of  wing  contractions  through  recruitment 
of  "fast-twitch"  motor  units  produces  a  dramatic  increase 
in  forward  propulsion  speed. 


Speed  changes  due  to  a  "change-of-gears" 

Centrally,  the  change-of-gears  from  slow  to  fast  swim- 
ming involves  reconfiguration  of  the  central  pattern  gen- 
erator (Arshavsky  et  ai.  1985d.  1989).  as  previously  in- 
active pedal  interneurons  become  active  elements  of  the 
swim  pattern  generator.  During  slow  swimming,  a  two- 
phase  motor  drive  is  produced  by  activity  in  two  antag- 
onistic groups  of  pedal  interneurons  that  interact  through 
reciprocal  inhibitory  connections  (Arshavsky  et  ai.  1985b, 


Figure  1 .  Intracellular  recording  from  a  pattern  generator  interneuron 
of  Clione  (top  trace)  with  a  simultaneous  extracellular  recording  from 
the  wing  nerve  (bottom  trace).  The  record  shows  a  change-of-gears  (arrow) 
involving  an  increase  in  cycle  frequency  and  a  tonic  depolarization  in 
the  interneuron.  The  change  recruits  large  spikes  in  the  wing  nerve  re- 
cording. These  large  spikes  have  been  shown  to  reflect  activity  in  large 
swim  motor  neurons.  Recording  by  A.  N.  Spencer.  University  of  Alberta. 


230 


R.  A.  SATTERLIE 


Rewiring  of  a  central  pattern  generator  is  certainly  not 
a  new  concept.  The  pyloric  central  pattern  generator  of 
the  lobster  stomatogastric  system  can  exhibit  at  least  four 
distinct  functional  circuits  and  thus  four  distinct  motor 
activities  (Flamm  and  Harris- Warrick.  1986a.  b;  Harris- 
Warrick  el  al,  1989).  In  addition  to  the  unmodulated 
circuit,  the  amine  modulators  dopamine.  octopamine,  and 
serotonin  can  each  produce  a  dramatically  distinct  func- 
tional circuit  (see  Harris- Warrick  el  al.,  1 989,  for  a  review). 
In  the  opisthobranch  mollusk  Tritonia.  variable  output 
in  the  body  wall  motor  systems  can  be  produced  by  vary- 
ing the  types  and  intensities  of  triggering  sensory  inputs. 
According  to  the  polymorphic  network  concept  (Getting 
and  Dekin,  1985).  different  inputs  can  activate  different 
configurations  of  motor  control  systems  to  produce 
unique  motor  outputs  as  distinctive  as  body  wall  with- 
drawal and  swimming  movements.  These  two  examples 
demonstrate  that  a  motor  control  system,  or  part  of  it, 
can  be  used  for  more  than  one  behavior.  In  comparison, 
reconfiguration  of  the  Clioneswim  pattern  generator  ap- 
pears to  involve  exclusively  frequency  modulation  rather 
than  changes  in  the  phase  relationships  or  functional  wir- 
ing of  the  pattern  generator. 

The  change  from  slow  to  fast  swimming  in  Clione  is 
induced  in  both  intact  and  reduced  preparations  when 
the  preparations  are  bathed  in  10~-  to  1CT6  M  serotonin 
(Arshavsky  el  al.,  1985a.  d:  Satterlie  1989).  Under  these 
conditions,  fast  swimming  continues  as  long  as  serotonin 
remains  in  the  bath.  At  the  level  of  individual  pattern 
generator  interneurons,  serotonin  produces  a  5-10  mV 
tonic  depolarization  similar  to  that  seen  during  sponta- 
neous fast  swimming.  The  source  of  these  tonic  depolar- 
izations is  not  known.  Serotonin  has  also  been  implicated 
in  the  initiation  of  swimming  activity  in  the  leech  (Kristan 
and  Weeks,  1983;  Nusbaum  and  Kristan  1986;  Nusbaum. 
1986)  and  of  Aplysia  brasiliana  (Parsons  and  Pinsker. 
1989),  as  well  as  pedal  locomotion  in  non-swimming 
Aplysia  (Mackey  and  Carew,  1983).  Serotonin  also  mod- 
ulates ongoing  rhythmic  activity  in  a  number  of  prepa- 
rations, including  lamprey  swimming  (Harris- Warrick  and 
Cohen,  1985),  feeding  in  Aplysia  (Kupfermann  and  Weiss, 
1982),  insect  flight  (Claassen  and  Kammer,  1986),  and 
the  pyloric  rhythm  of  the  lobster  (Flamm  and  Harris- 
Warrick,  1986a,  b).  Serotonin  can  also  have  system-wide 
behavioral  effects,  as  in  the  regulation  of  posture  in  lobsters 
(Kravitz  el  al.,  1985). 

Changes  of  swimming  speed  within  gears 

Although  the  possibilities  for  changes  of  speed  within 
gears  are  numerous,  four  possibilities  will  be  considered 
here:  ( 1 )  changes  in  tonic  input  to  swim  interneurons  and 
motor  neurons,  (2)  central  modulation  of  the  pattern  gen- 
erator (e.g.,  with  serotonergic  inputs)  at  a  level  not  suffi- 


cient to  change  gears,  (3)  the  role  of  endogenous  properties 
of  muscle  cells,  and  (4)  peripheral  modulation  of  muscle 
contractility.  The  first  two  involve  central  modifications 
while  the  last  two  modify  peripheral  activity. 

Changes  in  tonic  input  to  swim  neurons 

Despite  the  description  of  pedal  neurons  that  show 
variable  tonic  activity  associated  with  changes  in  pattern 
generator  activity  in  Clione  (Arshavsky  et  al.,  1984),  little 
is  known  about  the  variety  and  sources  of  tonic  influences 
over  pattern  generator  activity.  Inasmuch  as  tonic  depo- 
larization of  isolated  pattern  generator  interneurons  is  re- 
lated to  spontaneous  firing  frequency  (Arshavsky  et  al., 
1986),  then  tonic  inputs  can  presumably  modify  the  fre- 
quency of  pattern  generator  output.  Provided  that  the  in- 
puts do  not  cause  pattern  generator  reconfiguration,  the 
change  in  cycle  frequency  will  be  translated  into  a  change 
of  locomotory  speed  within  the  appropriate  "gear."  Tonic 
input  could  exert  this  influence  in  either  slow  or  fast 
swimming  gears. 

Central  modulation  not  sufficient  to  change  gears 

The  source  of  central  serotonergic  inputs  to  the  pattern 
generator  that  are  responsible  for  reconfiguration  and  gear 
change  have  not  yet  been  identified.  But  circumstantial 
evidence  now  in  hand  has  led  us  to  investigate  descending 
serotonergic  inputs  from  the  cerebral  ganglia.  Serotonin- 
immunoreactive  neurons  have  been  found  in  the  medial 
posterior  and  medial  anterior  regions  of  the  cerebral  gan- 
glia. Axons  from  some  of  these  cells  run  from  the  cerebral 
ganglia  to  the  pedal  ganglia  via  the  cerebro-pedal  con- 
nectives. Focal  extracellular  stimulation  of  the  medial 
posterior  region  of  a  pedal  ganglion  results  in  acceleration 
of  pattern  generator  activity,  or  with  strong  stimuli, 
changes  in  pattern  generator  activity  identical  to  changes 
associated  with  activation  of  fast  swimming  activity. 
Transection  of  the  cerebro-pedal  connective  greatly  re- 
duces these  responses.  Assuming  that  the  central  modu- 
lation does  not  operate  in  an  all-or-none  manner,  sub- 
threshold  levels  of  modulation  (subthreshold  for  change 
of  gears)  might  trigger  a  change  of  swimming  speed  within 
the  slow  gear,  and  different  levels  of  supra-threshold 
modulation  might  produce  variable  pattern  generator  ac- 
tivity in  the  fast  gear.  Such  changes  of  swimming  speed 
should  be  expressed  as  a  change  in  cycle  frequency,  unless 
swim  motor  neurons  are  also  affected  by  the  central  mod- 
ulatory  subsystem.  In  the  latter  case,  changes  in  both  cycle 
frequency  and  force  of  wing  movements  will  be  seen.  A 
further,  purely  speculative  possibility  allows  for  separate 
modulation  of  pattern  generator  interneurons  and  swim 
motor  neurons,  a  condition  that  would  add  greatly  to  the 
complexity  of  the  behavioral  output.  Potential  central 


LOCOMOTORY  SPEED  CHANGES 


231 


modulators  other  than  serotonin  are  not  being  considered 
here,  but  should  not  be  discounted. 

Intrinsic  properties  of  muscle  cells 

Intrinsic  properties  of  muscle  cells,  particularly  related 
to  repetitive  firing  activity,  can  influence  the  force  of  swim 
muscle  contractions.  Such  intrinsic  properties  could  be 
synaptic  or  non-synaptic,  the  latter  including  changes  in 
passive  or  active  membrane  properties,  or  in  excitation- 
contraction  coupling.  Both  slow-twitch  and  fast-twitch  fi- 
bers of  the  Clione  swimming  system  exhibited  non-syn- 
aptic facilitation  of  the  amplitude  of  spike-like  responses 
with  repetitive,  direct  depolarization  of  individual  muscle 
cells  (Satterlie,  1988).  The  facilitation  was  strongly  fre- 
quency-dependent, so  that  both  overall  amplitude  of 
spike-like  responses  and  initial  rate  of  change  of  spike- 
like  response  amplitude  showed  a  positive  correlation  with 
frequency  of  induced  activity  over  the  range  of  frequencies 
normally  encountered  during  slow  and  fast  swimming  (in 
prep.).  Provided  that  the  contractile  force  of  whole  muscles 
is  related  to  changes  in  spike-like  response  amplitude  re- 
corded from  individual  cells,  overall  muscle  force  should 
change  in  parallel  with  changes  in  pattern  generator  fre- 
quency. 

Peripheral  modulation  of  contractile  force 

A  cluster  of  7-10  serotonin-immunoreactive  neurons 
have  been  found  in  the  medial  margin  of  each  pedal  gan- 
glion of  Clione  (Fig.  2).  At  least  two  neurons  from  this 
cluster  send  axons  to  the  ipsilateral  wing  via  the  wing 
nerve.  Induced  activity  in  these  two  neurons  produced 
no  direct  motor  response:  but  when  activity  was  triggered 
during  ongoing  swimming  activity,  muscle  contractions 


Figure  2.  Schematic  diagram  of  the  dorsal  surface  of  the  left  pedal 
ganglion  of  Clione.  The  two  large  motor  neurons  (major  landmarks  of 
the  ganglion)  are  indicated  by  cells  1  and  2.  Cells  3  and  4  represent  motor 
neurons  that  initiate  escape  swimming.  The  remaining  cells  represent 
serotonin-immunoreactive  cells.  The  two  cells  marked  with  an  asterisk 
have  been  electrophysiologically  identified',  they  send  axons  into  the  ip- 
silateral wing  via  the  wing  nerve  (wn).  These  cells  enhance  muscle  con- 
tractility as  shown  in  Figure  3.  pc-pedal-pedal  commissure. 


Figure  3.  Dual  recording  from  a  serotonin-immunoreactive  neuron 
(bottom  trace)  and  a  wing  force  transducer  (top  trace — not  calibrated). 
Following  a  burst  of  action  potentials  in  the  neuron,  muscle  contractions 
are  enhanced.  The  latency  of  the  response  is  approximately  one  second, 
and  the  duration  is  5  s.  The  neuron  was  hyperpolarized  by  a  -1  nA 
current  during  the  recording  to  prevent  spiking.  The  burst  was  triggered 
by  switching  to  a  + 1  nA  current. 


were  enhanced  (Fig.  3).  The  response  latency  was  ap- 
proximately one  second  from  the  initiation  of  the  induced 
burst,  and  the  effect  lasted  from  3-10  s.  Preliminary  ev- 
idence suggests  that  this  enhancement  was  due  to  an  in- 
creased amplitude  of  the  spike-like  response  in  some,  but 
not  all,  of  the  muscle  cells. 

Peripheral  modulation,  including  both  pre-  and  post- 
synaptic  effects,  have  been  noted  in  numerous  prepara- 
tions (e.g..  Kravitz  et  at..  1985;  Kobayashi  and  Hasimoto, 
1982;  Maranto  and  Calabrese,  1984;  Weiss  et  ai,  1978). 
Induced  bursts  in  the  pedal  serotonin-immunoreactive 
neurons  of  Clione  produced  no  apparent  synaptic  activity 
in  either  pattern  generator  or  motor  neurons,  and  pro- 
duced no  changes  in  frequency  or  intensity  of  spike  ac- 
tivity in  either  neuron  type.  This  suggests  an  interesting 
dichotomy  in  serotonin  modulation  of  swimming  in 
Clione:  i.e..  pedal  serotonin-immunoreactive  neurons 
modulate  muscle  activity,  whereas  proposed  cerebral  se- 
rotonergic  neurons  modulate  pattern  generator  activity. 
A  similar  separation  of  central  and  peripheral  modulation 
is  seen  in  the  leech  heartbeat  system  (Calabrese  and  Arbas, 


Table  I 

Summary  of  four  possible  modulatory  states  in  the  swimming 
M'Wi'iH  "/  Clione  limacina  based  on  separate  central 
and  peripheral  modulatory  subsystems 


Modulatory  state 

No  modulation 


Peripheral  modulation  only 


Central  modulation  only 


Central  and  peripheral 
modulation 


Swimming  activity 

Slow  swimming,  normal  muscle 
contractility 

Slow  swimming,  enhanced  muscle 
contractility 

Fast  swimming,  normal  muscle 
contractility 

Fast  swimming,  enhanced  muscle 
contractility 


232 


R.  A.  SATTERLIE 


*f  f  f 


ImV 

0.5s 

*_ 


Figure  4.  Dual  recording  from  a  "startle"  motor  neuron  (bottom 
trace)  and  an  uncalibrated  wing  force  transducer  (top  trace).  Note  the 
absence  of  pattern  generator  input  to  the  neuron  despite  the  ongoing 
swimming  activity.  Bursts  of  action  potentials  were  triggered  in  the  neuron 
with  +12  nA  injected  currents  (through  the  recording  electrode).  The 
resultant  bursts  of  activity  induced  strong  contractions  of  the  wing. 


1985).  Assuming  the  simplest  case  of  supra-threshold 
modulation  in  both  central  and  peripheral  subsystems  of 
Clione.  the  separation  of  pattern  generator  and  muscle 
modulatory  subsystems  allows  four  possible  states  with 
respect  to  serotonin  modulation  of  swimming  activity 
(Table  I).  As  mentioned  previously,  central  modulation 
will  primarily  affect  cycle  frequency,  whereas  peripheral 
modulation  will  affect  contractile  force. 

This  discussion  takes  into  account  only  one  peripheral 
modulatory  system.  The  possibility  of  other  modulatory 
inputs,  as  well  as  the  release  of  multiple  transmitters  or 
modulators  from  the  serotonin-immunoreactive  neurons, 
could  add  further  complexity  to  the  swimming  system. 

Escape  swimming 

An  interesting  pair  of  motor  neurons  have  recently  been 
identified  from  each  pedal  ganglion  of  Clione  (Fig.  4). 
These  motor  neurons  activate  both  slow-twitch  and  fast- 
twitch  fibers  of  the  swim  musculature,  but  do  not  receive 
input  from  the  swim  pattern  generator.  The  neurons  were 
originally  overlooked,  because  they  are  electrically  silent 
during  normal  swimming  activity  and  have  extremely  high 
firing  thresholds.  In  some  preparations,  it  is  very  difficult 
to  stimulate  electrical  activity  from  these  cells  with  intra- 
cellular  current  injection.  Induced  bursts  of  spikes  in  the 
motor  neurons  produce  massive  contractions  of  the  ip- 
silateral  wing.  Despite  this  strong  peripheral  input,  the 
cells  have  no  inputs  to,  or  influence  over,  the  activity  of 
interneurons  of  the  pattern  generator  or  swim  motor  neu- 
rons. Their  powerful  effect  on  swim  musculature,  their 
total  independence  from  the  swim  pattern  generator,  and 
their  high  firing  threshold  suggest  that  these  motor  neurons 
may  participate  in  the  primary  phase  of  escape  swimming 
by  triggering  the  initial  ballistic  movement;  indeed,  the 
ballistic  movement  may  function  as  a  startle  response. 
The  maintenance  of  escape  swimming,  involving  the 
variable  period  of  enhanced  fast  swimming,  could  rep- 
resent activation  of  both  central  and  peripheral  seroton- 
ergic  modulatory  subsystems  (see  Table  I).  Multiple  re- 
cordings from  "startle"  neurons  and  serotonin-immu- 


noreactive neurons  following  tail  stimulation  in  intact 
preparations  have  not  yet  been  completed  due  to  technical 
difficulties,  but  should  help  clarify  this  relationship. 

The  foregoing  discussion  introduces  several  levels  at 
which  changes  of  locomotory  speed  can  occur  in  the 
swimming  system  of  Clione.  Some  of  the  results  are  pre- 
liminary, while  a  few  are  purely  speculative.  It  is  clear, 
however,  that  both  central  and  peripheral  modulatory  in- 
fluences are  operating,  and  that  significant  changes  in  both 
frequency  and  strength  of  wing  movements  can  contribute 
to  locomotory  speed  changes.  With  this  information,  we 
are  beginning  to  gain  an  appreciation  for  the  neurobio- 
logical  complexity  involved  in  locomotory  plasticity  in 
this  relatively  "simple"  swimming  system.  Our  compre- 
hension of  the  neuronal  bases  of  speed  changes  involves 
changes  of  gears,  changes  of  speed  within  gears,  and  su- 
perimposed inputs,  such  as  escape.  This  understanding  is 
providing  a  good  starting  point  for  further  investigation 
of  other  forms  of  input  and  modulation,  as  well  as  detailed 
descriptions  of  the  intrinsic  properties  of  all  cells  involved 
in  swimming  behavior. 

Acknowledgments 

I  thank  Lou  and  Alison  Satterlie  for  help  collecting 
experimental  animals.  Dr.  A.  O.  D.  Willows  for  providing 
space  and  facilities  at  Friday  Harbor  Laboratories,  and 
Dr.  A.  N.  Spencer  for  the  use  of  Figure  1 .  Research  covered 
in  this  paper  was  supported  by  a  National  Science  Foun- 
dation grant  (BNS85-1 1692)  and  a  research  grant  from 
the  Whitehall  Foundation. 

Literature  Cited 

Arshavsky,  Yu.I.,  I.  N.  Beloozerova,  G.  N.  Orlovsky,  G.  A.  Pavlova,  and 
Vu.V.  Panchin.  1984.  Neurons  of  pedal  ganglia  of  pteropodial  mol- 
lusc controlling  activity  of  the  locomotor  generator.  Nejrofiziologia 
16:  543-546. 

Arshavsky,  Yu.I.,  I.  N.  Beloozerova,  G.  N.  Orlovsky,  Yu.V.  Panchin, 
and  G.  A.  Pavlova.  I985a.  Control  of  locomotion  in  marine  mollusc 
Clume  limacina.  I.  Efferent  activity  during  actual  and  fictitious 
swimming.  Exp.  Brain  Res  58:  255-262. 

Arshavsky,  Yu.I.,  I.  N.  Beloozerova,  G.  N.  Orlovsky,  Yu.V.  Panchin, 
and  G.  A.  Pavlova.  1985b.  Control  of  locomotion  in  marine  mollusc 
Clione  limacina.  II.  Rhythmic  neurons  of  pedal  ganglia.  Exp.  Brain 
Rex.  58:  263-272. 

Arshavsky,  Yu.I.,  I.  N.  Beloozerova,  G.  N.  Orlovsky,  Yu.V.  Panchin, 
and  G.  A.  Pavlova.  I985c.  Control  of  locomotion  in  marine  mollusc 
Clione  limacina.  III.  On  the  origin  of  locomotory  rhythm.  Exp  Brain 
Res  58:  273-284. 

Arshavsky,  Yu.I.,  I.  N.  Beloozerova,  G.  N.  Orlovsky,  Yu.V.  Panchin, 
and  G.  A.  Pavlova.  1985d.  Control  of  locomotion  in  marine  mollusc 
Clione  I  i  mac  i  mi.  IV.  Role  of  type  12  interneurons.  Exp.  Brain  Res. 
58:  285-293. 

Arshavsky,  Yu.I.,  T.  G.  Beliagina,  G.  N.  Orlovsky,  Yu.V.  Panchin, 
G.  A.  Pavlova,  and  L.  B.  Popova.  1986.  Control  of  locomotion  in 
marine  mollusc  Clione  limacina.  VI.  Activity  of  isolated  neurons  of 
pedal  ganglia.  Exp.  Brain  Res.  63:  106-1 12. 


LOCOMOTORY  SPEED  CHANGES 


233 


Arshavsky,  Yu.l.,  0.  N.  Orlovsky,  Yu.V.  Panchin,  and  G.  A.  Pavlova. 
1989.  Control  of  locomotion  in  marine  mollusc  Clione  liniacina. 
VII.  Rcexamination  of  type  12  interneurons.  /L".Y/>  Brain  Res  78: 
398-406. 

Calabrese,  R.  L.,  and  E.  A.  Arbas.  1985.  Modulation  of  central  and 
peripheral  rhythmicity  in  the  heartbeat  system  of  the  leech.  Pp.  69- 
85  in  Model  Neural  Networks  and  Behavior.  A.  I.  Selverston,  ed. 
Plenum.  New  York. 

Claassen,  D.  E.,  and  A.  E.  Kammer.  1986.  Effects  of  octopamine.  do- 
pamine  and  serotonin  on  production  of  flight  motor  output  by  tho- 
racic ganglia  of  Manduca  sexta.  J  Neurobiol.  17:  1-14. 

Flamm,  R.  E.,  and  R.  INI.  Harris-Warrick.  1986a.  Aminergic  modu- 
lation in  the  lobster  stomatogastnc  ganglion.  I.  Effects  on  the  motor 
pattern  and  individual  neurons  within  the  pyloric  circuit.  ./,  Neiiro- 
physiol.  55:  847-865. 

Flamm,  R.  E.,  and  R.  M.  Harris- \\arrick.  1986b.  Aminergic  modu- 
lation in  the  lobster  stomatogastnc  ganglion.  II.  Target  neurons  of 
dopamine.  octopamine  and  serotonin  within  the  pyloric  circuit.  ./ 
Neurophysiol.  55:  866-881. 

Getting,  P.  A.,  and  M.  S.  Dekin.  1985.  Tritn/iui  swimming.  A  model 
system  for  integration  within  rhythmic  motor  systems.  Pp.  3-20  in 
Model  \eural  Networks  and  Behavior.  A.  I.  Selverston.  ed.  Plenum, 
New  York. 

Harris-Warrick.  R.  M.,  and  A.  H.  Cohen.  1985.  Serotonin  modulates 
the  central  pattern  generator  for  locomotion  in  the  isolated  lamprey 
spinal  cord.  J  Exp  Biol.  116:  27-46. 

Harris-Warrick,  R.  M.,  R.  E.  Flamm,  B.  R.  Johnson,  and  P.  S.  Katz. 
1989.  Modulation  of  neural  circuits  in  Crustacea.  Am  tool  29: 
1305-1320. 

Robayashi,  M.,  and  T.  Hasimoto.  1982.  Antagonistic  responses  of  the 
radular  protractor  and  retractor  to  the  same  putative  neurotrans- 
mitters.  (.'oiup  Hun-hem,  t'hysiol.  72C:  343-348. 

Kravitz,  E.  A.,  B.  Beltz,  S.  Glusman,  M.  Goy,  R.  Harris- Warrick,  M. 
Johnston,  M.  Livingstone,  T.  Schwarz,  and  K.  K.  Siwicki.  1985.  The 
well-modulated  lobster.  The  roles  of  serotonin,  octopamine  and 
proctolin  in  the  lobster  nervous  systems.  Pp.  339-360  in  Model  Neural 
Networks  and  Behavior.  A.  I.  Selverston,  ed.  Plenum,  New  York. 

Kristan.  \V.  B.,  Jr.,  and  J.  C.  Weeks.  1983.  Neurons  controlling  the 
initiation,  generation,  and  modulation  of  leech  swimming.  Pp.  243- 
260  in  Neural  Origin  oJ'Rhylhmie  Movements.  A.  Roberts  and  B.  L. 
Roberts,  eds.  Cambridge  University  Press,  Cambridge. 


Kupfermann,  I.,  and  k.  R.  Weiss.  1982.  Activity  of  an  identified  se- 
rotonergic  neuron  in  free  moving. -Iplysia  correlates  with  behavioral 
arousal.  Brain  Res  241:  334-337. 

Mackey,  S.,  and  I .  J.  Carew.  1983.  Locomotion  in  Aplysia:  triggering 
by  serotonin  and  modulation  by  bag  cell  extract.  /  Neumsci.  3:  1469- 
1477. 

Maranto,  A.  R.,  and  R.  L.  Calabrese.  1984.  Neural  control  of  the  hearts 
in  the  leech,  Hirudo  medicinalis  II.  Myogenic  activity  and  its  control 
by  heart  motor  neurons. ./.  Comp.  I'hysiol.  154:  381-391. 

Nusbaum,  M.  P.  1986.  Synaptic  basis  of  swim  initiation  in  the  leech. 
III.  Synaptic  effects  of  serotonin-containing  interneurones  (cells  21 
and  6 1 )  on  swim  CPG  neurones  (cells  1 8  and  208).  /  Exp.  Bio/.  1 22: 
303-321. 

Nusbaum,  M.  P.,  and  W.  B.  Kristan.  1986.  Swim  initiation  in  the  leech 
by  serotonin-containing  interneurones,  cells  2 1  and  6 1 .  J.  Exp.  Biol. 
122:277-302. 

Parsons,  D.  W.,  and  H.  M.  Pinsker.  1989.  Swimming  in  Aplysia  bras- 
i liana:  behavioral  and  cellular  effects  of  serotonin.  /.  Neurophysiol. 
62:  I  163-1176. 

Satterlie,  R.  A.  1985.  Reciprocal  inhibition  and  postinhibitory  rebound 
produce  reverberation  in  a  locomotor  pattern  generator.  Seience  229: 
402-404. 

Satterlie,  R.  A.  1987.  Neuromuscular  organization  for  two-speed 
swimming  in  a  pteropod  mollusc.  Six.  Neumsci.  Abslr.  13:  1061. 

Satterlie,  R.  A.  1988.  Peripheral  organization  of  the  swimming  system 
in  a  pteropod  mollusc.  Soc  Neurosci  Ahslr  14:  1001. 

Satterlie,  R.  A.  1989.  Reciprocal  inhibition  and  rhythmicity:  swimming 
in  a  pteropod  mollusc.  Pp.  151-171  in  Neuronal  and  Cellular  Os- 
cillators. J.  W.  Jacklet,  ed.  Dekker.  New  York. 

Satterlie,  R.  A.,  and  A.  N.  Spencer.  1985.  Swimming  in  the  pteropod 
mollusc.  Clione  linuicinu  II.  Physiology./  Exp.  Biol.  116:205-222. 

Satterlie,  R.  A.,  M.  LaBarbera,  and  A.  N.  Spencer.  1985.  Swimming 
in  the  pteropod  mollusc  C/ione  liinaeinu.  I.  Behaviour  and  mor- 
phology. /  Exp  Biol.  116:  184-204. 

Satterlie,  R.  A.,  G.  E.  Goslow  Jr.,  and  A.  Reyes.  1990.  Two  types  of 
striated  muscle  suggest  two-geared  swimming  in  the  pteropod  mollusc 
Clione  lunaana.  J  E.v/>.  /<«•/.  255:  131-140. 

Weiss,  K.  R.,  J.  L.  Cohen,  and  I.  Kupfermann.  1978.  Modulatory  con- 
trol of  buccal  musculature  by  a  serotonergic  neuron  (metacerebral 
cell)  tn  Aplysia.  J  Neurophysiol  41:  181-203. 


Reference:  Bwi  Bull.  180:  234-240.  (April,  1991) 


On  the  Significance  of  Neuronal  Giantism 

in  Gastropods 


RHANOR  GILLETTE 

Department  of  Physiology  &  Biophysics  and  The  Neuroscience  Program,  524  Burrill  Hall, 
407  S.  Goodwin  Ave.,  University  of  Illinois,  Urbana,  Illinois  61801 


Abstract.  Neurons  of  the  central  ganglia  of  opistho- 
branch  and  pulmonate  gastropods  increase  in  size  as  the 
animals  grow,  some  becoming  veritable  giants.  The  origins 
and  functions  of  neuronal  giantism  are  considered  here 
from  a  comparative  viewpoint.  A  review  of  the  properties 
of  identified  neurons  in  a  variety  of  opisthobranch  and 
pulmonate  species  indicates  that  neuronal  size  is  directly 
related  to  the  extent  of  postsynaptic  innervation.  DNA 
endoreplication,  resulting  in  partial  or  complete  poly- 
ploidy,  supports  giantism  in  molluscan  neurons  as  it  does 
in  eukaryotic  cells  elsewhere.  Apparently,  the  functional 
significance  of  giantism  is  enhanced  synthesis  and  trans- 
port of  materials  to  serve  an  expanded  presynaptic  func- 
tion. 

Giant  neurons  are  found  in  larger  snails  where  they 
innervate  large  areas  of  the  periphery;  interneurons  and 
sensory  neurons  are  enlarged  to  a  lesser  degree,  probably 
to  that  which  enables  load-matching  to  the  peripheral  ef- 
fectors. Neuronal  giantism  may  be  an  adaptation  for  the 
innervation  of  the  periphery  in  large  animals  with  simple 
behaviors  and  uncomplex  sensoria,  this  adaptation  en- 
abling growth  of  body  and  CNS  without  a  proportionate 
increase  in  neuronal  number.  A  more  complete  under- 
standing of  the  evolutionary  and  adaptive  significance  of 
neuronal  giantism  should  be  sought  in  comparative  studies 
of  the  cellular  properties  of  simple  and  complex  molluscan 
brains. 

Introduction 

The  condition  of  neuronal  giantism  in  the  pulmonate 
and  opisthobranch  gastropods  has  been  a  point  of  marvel 
at  least  since  Buchholz'  observations  in  1863  (reviewed 
by  Bullock,  1965).  The  conveniences  offered  by  giant 

Received  7  August  1990;  accepted  25  January  1991. 


nerve  cells  to  experimenters  have  also  invited  numerous 
biophysical  and  neuroethological  studies;  these  have  con- 
tributed greatly  to  our  knowledge  of  nerve  cell  function 
and  behavioral  mechanisms.  Even  so,  the  significance  of 
neuronal  giants  to  the  animals  in  which  they  are  found 
has  not  been  satisfactorily  understood. 

The  question  of  neuronal  giantism  is  particularly  open 
to  the  methods  of  comparative  analysis.  The  physiology, 
anatomy,  and  behavioral  roles  of  giant  neurons  have  been 
analyzed  from  a  wide  variety  of  species,  and  homologous 
neurons  have  been  identified  across  species.  The  following 
paragraphs  marshal  evidence  that  supports  several  hy- 
potheses for  the  origin  and  functional  significance  of  neu- 
ronal giantism. 

The  Molluscan  Neuron 

The  typical  molluscan  neuron  is  a  monopolar  or  bipolar 
cell  with  its  soma  lying  in  the  ganglion  periphery  (Fig.  1 ). 
An  axon  enters  the  neuropil  in  the  core  of  the  ganglion 
where  it  branches  off  neurites  that  both  receive  and  make 
synaptic  contacts.  Neurites  generally  sprout  close  to  the 
cell  body  and  even  originate  from  it  in  opisthobranch  and 
pulmonate  neurons.  Action  potentials  are  initiated  in  the 
axon  and  regulated  by  synaptic  inputs  to  the  neurites;  the 
region  of  spike  initiation  and  synaptic  activity  is  referred 
to  here  as  the  integrating  region. 

Neuronal  Giantism:  The  Condition 

The  condition  of  "giantism"  is  one  of  degree.  The  cen- 
tral ganglia  of  opisthobranch  and  pulmonate  snails  com- 
monly possess  10-20  distinct  and  identifiable  nerve  cells 
with  cell  bodies  so  large  that  they  stand  out  from  their 
neighbors  as  relative  giants.  Aside  from  the  obvious  giant 
neurons,  the  entire  central  nervous  system  of  such  animals 
contains  only  several  tens  of  thousands  of  neurons,  several 


234 


GIANT  NEURONS  IN  SNAILS 


235 


cbc 


Figure  1.  Typical  morphology  of  giant  neurons  of  pulmonates  and 
opisthobranchs,  as  exemplified  by  this  drawing  of  the  serotonergic  giant 
of  the  cerebral  ganglion  of  Tntuniu  hombcrgi  (from  Dorset!.  1986).  The 
large  excitable  soma  is  close  to  the  integrating  region  of  axon  and  fine 
neuntes,  where  synaptic  potentials  occur  and  spikes  are  initiated.  The 
large  axons,  with  high  specific  membrane  resistances,  favor  current  spread 
from  integrating  region  to  soma. 


hundred  of  which  may  be  identified  on  the  basis  of  po- 
sition, color,  synaptic  and  axonal  connections  (<.;/.  Bullock. 
1965;  Coggeshall.  1967;  Frazier  ct  ai.  1967).  In  the  larger 
pulmonates,  the  biggest  neurons  have  somata  approaching 
100  /urn  in  diameter,  whereas  in  the  larger  sea  slugs,  certain 
neuronal  somata  reach  over  700-800  j/m.  Moreover,  as 
the  animals  increase  in  size,  all  of  their  identifiable  neurons 
also  grow  in  diameter. 

Neuronal  Size  is  Related  to  Postsynaptic  Innervation 

In  approaching  the  nature  of  neuronal  giantism,  the 
first  relevant  observation  is  that  neuron  giants  must  in- 
nervate larger  postsynaptic  target  areas  than  non-giants. 
The  evidence  that  neuronal  size  is  directly  related  to  the 
extent  of  postsynaptic  innervation  comes  from  the  liter- 
ature characterizing  a  variety  of  identified  neurons  in 
opisthobranch  and  pulmonate  snails.  The  largest  neurons 
of  the  central  ganglia  act  as  effectors  that  innervate  large 
areas  of  the  periphery. 

Prominent  examples  are  a  bilateral  pair  of  giant  sero- 
tonergic neurons  identified  in  many  opisthobranch  and 
pulmonate  snail  species.  These  neurons  are  commonly 
the  largest  neuronal  somata  of  the  cerebral  ganglion 
(Senseman  and  Gelperin,  1973;  Berry  and  Pentreath, 
1976;  Weiss  and  Kupfermann,  1976;  Gillette  and  Davis, 
1977;  Granzow  and  Kater,  1977).  Approaching  400-500 
jim  in  size  in  the  larger  opisthobranchs,  these  giant  effec- 
tors send  large  axons  down  the  cerebrobuccal  connectives; 
the  axons  ramify  within  the  buccal  ganglion  so  that  an 
axonal  branch  is  sent  out  in  each  nerve.  These  axons  in- 
nervate large  areas  of  the  muscular  buccal  mass  and  the 
esophagus;  the  neurons  also  send  branches  out  the  lip  or 
mouth  nerves  of  the  cerebral  ganglion  to  innervate  the 
oral  region  (Fig.  2).  In  addition,  the  giant  serotonergic 
neurons  have  some  synaptic  output  in  the  buccal  ganglia 
(ibid.). 

Other  well-studied  giants  are  two  of  the  largest  neurons 
known,  the  neurons  R2  and  LP11  of  the  anaspid  opis- 


thobranch Aplysia  California.  R2  and  LP1 1  are  bilaterally 
homologous  and  cholinergic,  attaining  soma  diameters 
nearly  1000  nm  in  large  animals.  Due  to  assymetrical 
ganglionic  fusion  in  the  embryo,  R2  is  found  in  the  ab- 
dominal ganglion,  and  LP11  in  the  left  parietal  ganglion. 
The  cell  bodies  give  off  giant  axons  that  send  branches  to 
most  ganglia  and  out  many  nerves  thence  innervating  ex- 
tensive areas  of  the  skin  (Hughes  and  Tauc,  1963;  Cobbs 
and  Pinsker,  1979).  Their  electrical  activity  stimulates 
mucus  secretion  (Rayport  el  ai.  1983). 

Among  the  motorneurons  innervating  the  gills  of  nu- 
dibranchs  and  notaspids  are  some  of  the  largest  neurons 
of  the  pedal,  pleural,  and  cerebral  ganglia  (Blackshaw  and 
Dorsett,  1976;  Dickinson,  1979,  1980). 

The  well-studied  buccal  ganglia  provide  more  examples 
of  giantism.  The  largest  neurons  of  opisthobranch  buccal 


Figure  2.  Extensive  innervation  of  the  periphery  by  the  serotonergic 
cerebral  giant  neurons  of  Helix  pomalia  (from  Berry  and  Pentreath. 
1974).  Aside  from  some  interneuronal  function  in  the  CNS,  these  giants 
send  many  branches  to  the  buccal  ganglion  and  out  the  nerves  to  innervate 
the  musculature  of  the  buccal  mass  and  esophagus.  Other  branches  leave 
anterior  nerves  to  innervate  the  feeding  musculature  of  the  oral  region. 
This  general  plan  is  found  in  the  homologous  giant  cells  of  many  pul- 
monate and  opisthobranch  species. 


236 


R.  GILLETTE 


ganglia  are  typically  motorneurons:  sensory  neurons  are, 
on  the  average,  much  smaller  (Byrne  el  a/.,  1974;  Siegler, 
1977;  Spray  el  a/.,  1980;  Dorsett  and  Sigger,  1981).  The 
largest  known  buccal  cells  may  be  those  of  the  buccal 
ganglion  of  the  cephalaspid  Nuvanax.  These  neurons  in- 
nervate the  musculature  of  the  large  pharynx,  driving  its 
expansion  during  prey-capture  (Spira  and  Bennett,  1972). 
In  aeolid  and  doridacean  buccal  ganglia,  the  largest  neu- 
rons are  often  a  bilateral  pair  called  the  Dorsal  White 
Cells  (Bulloch  and  Dorsett,  1979).  The  Dorsal  White  Cells 
are  peptidergic  neurons  that  send  axons  out  the  gastro- 
esophageal  nerve  to  ramify  over,  and  innervate,  the  large 
esophagus  (Masinovsky  and  Lloyd,  1985). 

Interneurons  with  only  central  synaptic  outputs  tend 
to  be  smaller  than  interneurons  of  dual  function,  i.e..  with 
both  CNS  output  and  peripheral  axons  innervating  mus- 
cle. For  instance,  both  the  identified  VWC  and  B3I  neu- 
rons of  Pleurobranchaea  can  drive  intense  cyclic  motor 
output  in  the  buccal  oscillator  network;  but  the  VWC  also 
innervates  the  muscular  esophagus,  and  the  diameter  of 
its  soma  is  nearly  three  times  that  of  the  B3I  soma  (Gillette 
elal.  1980).  Identified  neurons  with  purely  central  outputs 
also  may  differ  in  size  according  to  the  extent  of  their 
postsynaptic  output.  The  paired  SO  interneurons  of  the 
buccal  ganglion  of  the  pulmonate  Lymnaea  have  a  large 
dendritic  field,  and  their  somata  are  three  times  the  size 
of  the  interneurons  of  the  Nl,  N2,  and  N3  populations, 
which  have  collectively  rather  similar  functions  as  oscil- 
lator elements,  but  smaller  dendritic  fields  in  the  ganglion 
and  weaker  effects,  individually,  on  the  network  (Elliot 
and  Benjamin,  1985a,  b). 

Sensory  neurons  can  innervate  large  peripheral  areas, 
but  their  presynaptic  function  is  largely  confined  to  central 
ganglia,  and  they  tend  to  be  small.  Sensory  neurons  of 
the  buccal  ganglia  of  Pleurobranchaea  tend  to  have  smaller 
somata  than  motorneurons  innervating  the  same  muscles 
of  the  buccal  mass  (Siegler,  1977).  Similarly,  the  buccal 
ganglia  of  Navanax  contain  mechanosensory  neurons  that 
serve  the  pharynx  and  are  much  smaller  than  their  post- 
synaptic giant  motorneurons  that  drive  the  pharyngeal 
musculature  (Spray  el  ai,  1980a,  b).  Sensory  neurons  car- 
rying mechanosensory  information  from  the  skin  of  Tri- 
tonia  are  quite  smaller  than  the  interneurons  and  motor- 
neurons  they  drive  (Getting,  1977).  The  abdominal  gan- 
glion ofApIysia  contains  sensory  neurons  that  innervate 
the  gill  and  siphon  and  that  are  much  smaller  than  the 
gill  and  siphon  motorneurons  they  drive  (Byrne  el  al., 
1974).  In  each  case,  the  sensory  neurons  have  smaller 
dendritic  fields,  and  thus  may  make  fewer  synaptic  con- 
tacts, than  the  larger  motorneurons  and  interneurons. 

A  direct  relationship  between  the  field  of  postsynaptic 
innervation  of  a  neuron  and  its  soma  size  has  been  pre- 
viously recognized  by  some  workers  in  arthropod  neu- 
robiology.  Mittenthal  and  Wine  (1978)  showed  that  the 


soma  diameter  of  serially  homologous  motorneurons  in 
the  segmental  nervous  system  of  crayfish  is  roughly  pro- 
portional to  the  area  of  the  serially  homologous  muscle 
they  innervate.  Mellon  el  al.  (1981)  showed  that  ampu- 
tation of  the  specialized  snapping  claw  of  the  snapping 
shrimp  Alpheus  causes  the  contralateral  claw  and  its  mus- 
culature to  enlarge  into  a  larger  snapping  claw  at  subse- 
quent molts;  the  soma  of  the  claw  opener  motorneuron 
enlarges  with  the  size  of  its  target  organ. 

Finally,  the  peripheral  effector  neurons  of  the  opis- 
thobranch  central  nervous  system  increase  in  size  with 
the  growth  of  their  target  organs.  The  size  of  identified 
neurons,  in  soma  diameter,  axon  diameter,  and  dendritic 
field,  increases  with  the  size  of  the  animal  during  growth 
(Coggeshall,  1967;  Frazier  el  al.,  1967).  Accordingly,  sen- 
sory interneurons  monitoring  the  peripheral  effectors  and 
the  smaller  interneurons  also  increase  in  size;  this  is  a 
form  of  load  matching.  All  of  these  observations  argue 
for  a  trophic  relationship  between  the  area  of  the  inner- 
vated structure  and  the  size  of  the  presynaptic  neuron.  It 
is  assumed  here,  notwithstanding  the  lack  of  direct  evi- 
dence, that  increases  in  innervated  area  and  extent  of  pre- 
synaptic branching  are  accompanied  by  increases  in  syn- 
aptic contact  area,  number  of  synaptic  sites,  or  both. 
Therefore,  the  beginning  of  the  answer  to  the  question: 
"Why  do  some  neurons  become  giants?"  is  probably  that 
their  size  is  related  to  the  actual  total  area  of  synaptic 
contact. 

The  Mechanism  of  Giantism:  DNA  Endoreplication 

For  certain  cell  types  in  many  animals,  an  increase  in 
cell  size  is  generally  accompanied  by  an  increase  in  the 
actual  mass  of  the  genomic  DNA  and  of  RNA  (Mirsky 
andOsawa,  1 96 1 ;  r/.  Cavalier-Smith,  1978);  this  is  effected 
either  through  polyploidy  or  polyteny.  An  increase  in 
polyploidy  with  neuronal  size  has  been  demonstrated  in 
molluscan  neurons.  The  nuclei  of  the  largest  neurons  of 
mature  Aplysia  (e.g..  R2)  contain  >0.2  n%  of  DNA — more 
than  200,000  times  the  haploid  amount  (Lasek  and 
Dower,  1971).  Neurons  of  the  terrestrial  pulmonate 
Achatina,  with  soma  diameters  of  >9  nm  (nuclear  di- 
ameter >  7  ^m),  were  found  to  be  polyploid  (Chase  and 
Tolloczko,  1987).  The  frequency  distribution  of  the  DNA 
content  in  Achatina  (Chase  and  Tollockzo,  1987)  and 
Planorbis  (Lombardo  et  al..  1980)  neurons  indicates  that 
endoreplication  during  growth  probably  represents  selec- 
tive gene  amplification,  rather  than  simple  sequential 
doubling.  However,  sequential  doubling  may  occur  during 
growth  in  Aplysia  (Coggeshall  et  al..  1970;  Lasek  and 
Dower,  1971).  Giantism  in  molluscan  neurons  is  thus 
like  giantism  in  other  metazoan  cells,  and  is  simply  based 
on  increased  amounts  of  nucleic  acids  and  proteins. 

Polyploid  neurons  of  varying  sizes  may  be  common  to 
the  nervous  systems  of  molluscs  in  general;  i.e..  increasing 


GIANT  NEURONS  IN  SNAILS 


237 


neuron  size  and  ploidy  may  be  a  usual  feature  of  growth 
within  all  of  the  molluscan  classes;  one  that  is,  perhaps, 
carried  to  the  extreme  in  the  pulmonates  and  opistho- 
branchs. 

The  Functions  of  Giantism:  Synthesis  and  Transport 

Neuronal  giants  apparently  innervate  larger  postsyn- 
aptic  target  areas  than  non-giants.  Neuronal  giantism, 
therefore,  may  allow  an  increase  in  animal  size  without 
a  proportional  increase  in  the  number  of  central  neurons. 
Giant  cells  in  most  tissues  are  more  metabolically  active 
than  smaller  cells  and  are  frequently  associated  with 
transport  and  secretory  processes.  Familiar  examples  are 
the  giant  polytene  cells  of  dipteran  salivary  glands,  mal- 
pighian  tubules,  and  gut,  all  of  which  are  notably  active 
in  ion  and  peptide  transport  and  exocytotic  secretion. 
Thus,  elaboration  of  DNA,  RNA.  and  protein  in  many 
giant  cells  is  indicative  of  enhanced  synthetic  capacity, 
presumably  to  serve  the  needs  of  increased  cell  activity. 
In  giant  neurons,  these  needs  are  likely  to  be  connected 
with  increased  axon  transport  and  secretion  processes  at 
their  extensively  distributed  synaptic  terminals. 

Thus,  the  picture  of  the  giant  neuron  becomes  one 
where  the  size,  synthetic  capacity,  and  axonal  transport 
traffic  is  adapted  to  the  extent  of  postsynaptic  innervation. 
The  giant  cells  do  the  work  of  many  smaller  cells  in  other 
nervous  systems. 

The  Evolutionary  Origin  and  Integrative  Significance 
of  Neuronal  Giantism  in  Gastropods 

The  occurrence  of  giant  neurons  in  snails  is  explained 
in  one  sense  by  the  observation  that  the  giant  neurons 
must  innervate  large  postsynaptic  areas.  The  imposing 
question  that  looms  is:  why  do  the  pulmonates  and  opis- 
thobranchs  display  such  pronounced  neuron  giantism 
whereas  other  gastropod  taxa  do  not?  The  best  answer 
will  probably  rest  on  future  comparative  observations  on 
species  chosen  for  particular  nervous  system  characters, 
but  the  context  for  such  comparative  observations  can  be 
set  here.  The  approach  is  to  enumerate  the  specific  set  of 
behavioral  and  neurophysiological  characteristics  that 
may  place  the  opisthobranch/pulmonate  line  apart  from 
other  gastropods:  in  the  process,  perhaps,  a  few  useful 
speculations  may  be  generated. 

Those  gastropods  that  are  distinguished  by  possession 
of  a  score  or  more  of  large  neurons  are  also  distinguished 
by  the  combination  of  the  following  characteristics: 

1 .  relatively  large  body  size; 

2.  motile,  foraging  lifestyles  sustained  by  relatively  simple 
behavior; 

3.  simple  nervous  systems  lacking,  for  the  most  part, 
complex  sensoria; 


4.  a  fairly  high  degree  of  centralization  within  the  CNS; 
and 

5.  excitable  neuron  cell  bodies. 

Although  one  or  more  of  these  characteristics  may  appear 
in  various  gastropod  taxa,  the  appearance  of  all  five  may 
be  relatively  specific  to  the  opisthobranch/pulmonate  line. 

The  gastropods  crept  into  the  fossil  record  around  580 
million  years  ago  as  minute  animals  1-2  mm  in  shell 
diameter,  and  today  most  are  still  smaller  than  5  mm. 
The  larger  modern  gastropods  are  thus  truly  somato- 
morphic  giants;  their  greater  body  size  demands  enhanced 
innervation  of  the  periphery.  In  most  large  species,  this 
need  is  met  largely  by  an  increase  in  brain  size  and  neuron 
number:  even  in  the  opisthobranch/pulmonate  line,  the 
number  of  neurons  (and  the  number  of  peripheral  axons) 
increases  with  body  size,  in  parallel  with  the  striking  in- 
crease in  size  of  identified  neurons  (Coggeshall,  1967). 
But  if,  as  has  been  argued,  giant  neurons  are  an  adaptation 
for  increased  area  of  innervation,  then  during  evolution 
these  snails  have  made  a  trade  of  neuron  size  for  neuron 
number  in  the  innervation  of  an  enlarging  periphery.  This 
trade  has  apparently  not  been  made  by  the  other  larger 
gastropods  belonging  to  the  prosobranchs. 

Large  body  size  in  gastropods  is  associated  with  a  mo- 
tile, foraging  lifestyle,  as  opposed  to  the  sedentary  life  of 
a  parasite  or  filter  feeder.  Motile  foragers  are  generally 
expected  to  exhibit  a  certain  complexity  in  their  behavior, 
complexity  that  would  emerge  from  corresponding  com- 
plexity in  the  nervous  system.  However.  I  suggest  that  the 
behavior  of  the  opisthobranchs  and  pulmonates,  relative 
to  that  of  the  larger  advanced  prosobranchs,  is  both  sim- 
pler and  underlain  by  a  simpler  nervous  system. 

CNS  development  is  directly  associated  with  sensory 
and  behavioral  ability.  The  behavior  of  opisthobranchs 
and  pulmonates,  like  their  nervous  systems,  probably  lacks 
the  complexity  shown  by  the  larger  prosobranch  snails; 
the  number  of  behavioral  sub-routines  they  use  in  daily 
living  is  obviously  smaller  than  those  of  animals  living  in 
more  complex  ecological  niches.  Larger,  more  complex 
brains,  with  large  numbers  of  small  neurons,  are  associated 
with  the  development  of  sense  organs  for  high-resolution 
analysis  of  the  environment  and  greater  complexity  of 
behavior.  In  the  predatory  prosobranch  whelks,  the  many 
tiny  neurons,  relatively  large  ganglia,  and  eyes  are  likely 
to  mediate  similarly  complex  behaviors.  The  whelk  Fu- 
sitriton  oregonensis  devotes  considerable  behavioral 
strategy  to  reproduction.  Mating  pairs  form  seasonally 
and  persist  for  as  long  as  4  months.  Subsequently,  a  parent 
attaches  its  clutch  of  eggs  to  a  rock  surface  and  patrols 
them  against  predators  (Eaton,  1972).  Potential  predators 
may  be  sensed  in  part  by  the  whelk's  well-developed  eyes; 
the  whelk,  with  twisting  movements  of  its  shell,  attempts 
to  attack  and  dislodge  the  preditor;  failing  that,  the  whelk 


238 


R.  GILLETTE 


may  directionally  squirt  an  aversive  acid  secretion.  The 
opisthobranchs  and  pulmonates,  with  their  rudimentary- 
at-best  vision  and  small  numbers  of  CNS  neurons,  come 
nowhere  near  such  complexity  of  behavior.  Indeed,  the 
behavior  of  the  opisthobranchs  and  pulmonates  really 
seems  simple. 

The  relative  lack  of  complex  sensoria  and  their  atten- 
dant complex  central  processing  may  allow  the  opistho- 
branch/pulmonate  lines  to  live  successfully  with  a  highly 
reduced  CNS.  Their  eyes  are  very  small  and  quite  limited 
in  both  the  number  and  resolution  of  photoreceptors;  in 
many  opisthobranch  species,  the  eyes  are  even  internal- 
ized. Their  function  may  be  largely  limited  to  setting  the 
circadian  rhythms  of  animal  activity  (Jacklet,  1969).  High 
resolution  eyes  in  the  cephalopods  are  associated  with 
comparably  high  resolution,  visually  directed  motor  be- 
havior (cf.  Wells,  1978).  High  resolution  in  sensory-motor 
systems  requires  larger  numbers  of  neurons,  as  are  found 
in  the  cephalopod  optic  lobes.  The  opisthobranchs  get 
along  mostly  with  the  environmental  information  pro- 
vided by  chemosensory  and  tactile  abilities.  The  opis- 
thobranchs and  pulmonates  do  have  specialized  chemo- 
sensory sites  for  detecting  food:  the  rhinophores.  and  the 
tentacles  and  other  regions  about  the  oral  area.  These  sites 
appear  to  be  served  by  peripheral  ganglia  that  may  take 
the  burden  of  a  great  deal  of  sensory-motor  processing 
(cf.,  Mpitsos  and  Lukowiak,  1986).  leaving  the  central 
nervous  system  to  process  simple  tactile  information  and 
to  integrate  motivational  and  learning  processes  with  the 
expression  of  behavior. 

Contrasting  examples  support  this  interpretation.  Some 
pulmonates  and  prosobranchs  have  developed  accessory 
CNS  ganglionic  lobes;  these  structures  are  associated  with 
chemosensation  and  are  composed  of  many  smaller  neu- 
rons (cf.  Bullock,  1965;  Chase  and  Tolloczko,  1989).  In 
the  terrestrial  slug  Umax,  the  structure  is  the  procerebral 
lobe,  and  it  shows  oscillating  electrical  field  potentials 
characteristic  of  rather  complex  sensory  feature  extraction 
systems  in  vertebrates  (Gelperin  and  Tank,  1990).  Outside 
of  the  gastropods,  the  obvious  example  is  the  complexity 
of  sensoria  and  sensory  processing  in  the  complex  brains 
of  cephalopods.  In  the  opisthobranchs  and  pulmonates, 
the  lack  of  complexity  in  sensoria  and  underlying  neural 
processing  underscores  their  simplicity  of  brain  and  life- 
style. 

Finally,  the  opisthobranch  and  pulmonate  nervous 
systems  show  a  relatively  high  degree  of  centralization 
into  a  few  discrete  ganglia.  Although  centralization  and 
cephalization  have  not  proceeded  as  far  as  in  the  cepha- 
lopods, these  characteristics  still  distinguish  them  from 
the  mostly  sessile  bivalves  and  the  parasitic  or  filter-feeding 
gastropods  that  rely  heavily  on  peripheral  control  of  re- 
flexes and  show  generally  less  centralization.  It  also  dis- 
tinguishes them  from  large,  motile  mollusks  like  the  giant 


chitons,  from  which  giant  neurons  are  not  reported.  The 
chitons  attain  large  size  in  some  cases,  and  are  slowly 
motile,  but  their  nervous  system  is  only  poorly  centralized 
relative  to  that  of  the  opisthobranchs  and  pulmonates. 
Amphineuran  ganglia  are  simply  formed  nodes  lying  on 
a  major  nerve  ring,  and  many  neuronal  somata  are  simply 
dispersed  along  the  nerves  and  connectives  in  a  primitive 
medullary  condition  (cf.  Bullock,  1965). 

Thus,  snails  with  giant  neurons  constitute  a  group  that 
has  grown  large  in  body  size,  but  has  retained  an  uncom- 
plicated behavioral  repertory  and  sensory-motor  capaci- 
ties. To  serve  the  innervation  needs  of  the  enlarged  body, 
the  nervous  system  has  favored  an  increase  in  neuron  size 
relative  to  neuron  number. 

The  above  characters  provide  the  context  within  which 
I  think  we  must  seek  the  evolutionary  reasons  that  some 
snails  chose  the  architecture  of  neuronal  giantism  in  their 
nervous  systems.  Why  didn't  they  choose  instead  to  in- 
nervate their  enlarged  periphery  with  many  smaller  central 
neurons,  like  their  large  prosobranch  cousins?  The  de- 
velopmental simplicity  of  innervating  a  large  area  with 
one  neuron  rather  than  many  may  be  a  useful  consider- 
ation. Another  is  that  neuronal  giantism  allows  increased 
animal  size  without  a  proportional  increase  in  central 
neuron  number. 

A  potential  answer  lies  in  the  electrophysiological 
properties  of  neurons  of  simple  and  complex  gastropod 
brains.  In  molluscan  ganglia,  the  neurons  lie  peripherally 
and  send  axons  into  a  central  neuropil  to  make  synaptic 
connections.  For  the  opisthobranch  and  pulmonate  snails 
the  neuronal  somata  are  excitable  and  are  placed  spatially 
and  electrically  quite  close  to  the  integrating  region  (Gor- 
man and  Mirolli,  1972;  Graubard,  1975);  they  are  thus 
able  to  follow  almost  synchronously  the  spike  activity  of 
the  integrating  region  (cf.  Fig.  1).  In  larger  ganglia,  the 
distances  from  the  synaptic  integrating  region  in  the  neu- 
ropil, where  spikes  are  initiated,  to  soma  also  become 
longer.  Thus,  for  a  neuron  that  grows  in  pace  with  the 
whole  ganglion,  the  larger  axon  diameter  would  enhance 
the  synchrony  of  soma  and  integrating  region.  In  this 
manner,  the  continuing  enlargement  of  neuronal  somata 
with  the  growth  of  the  organism  may  not  only  adapt  the 
cells  to  the  innervation  of  an  enlarged  periphery,  but  also 
to  an  increased  separation  of  the  soma  from  the  integrating 
region. 

Some  data  suggest  that  the  very  small  neuronal  somata 
of  advanced  cephalopod  ganglia  are  inexcitable  (Gilly  and 
Brismar,  1989;  Williamson  and  Budelmann,  1991;  Rob- 
ertson et  a/..  1990),  like  those  of  arthropods.  Many  of 
these  somata  lie  rather  distant  from  the  neuropil  inte- 
grating regions,  because  they  are  packed  on  top  of  many 
intervening  cells  in  the  cell  body  layer  (cf.  Young,  1971). 
For  a  small  neuron,  this  longer  distance  could  cause  a 
disadvantageous  desynchronization  of  the  action  potential 


GIANT  NEURONS  IN  SNAILS 


239 


currents  between  the  soma  and  integrating  region,  with 
the  result  that  the  late  somatic  currents  would  interfere 
with  ongoing  integration  (in  the  worst  case,  by  reflecting 
spikes).  Thus,  for  the  nervous  systems  having  a  high  pro- 
portion of  such  small  neurons,  it  might  be  functionally 
advantageous  if  the  cellular  mechanisms  of  somatic  ex- 
citability were  turned  off.  We  would  then  wonder,  in  gen- 
eral, whether  large  brains  with  many  small  neurons  have 
inexcitable  somata.  Do  gastropods,  such  as  the  larger 
whelks  with  large  central  ganglia  and  many  small  neuron 
somata  distant  from  the  integrating  neuropil,  have  spiking 
or  non-spiking  somata?  Few  intracellular  recordings  have 
been  made  in  the  complex  nervous  systems  of  the  ad- 
vanced giant  prosobranchs,  but  if  their  small  neuron  so- 
mata were  also  inexcitable,  a  strong  case  could  be  made 
that  the  simplicity  and  small  neuronal  numbers  of  the 
opisthobranch/pulmonate  central  nervous  system  permits 
the  retention,  in  evolution,  of  excitable  somata  with  an 
increase  in  the  size  of  ganglia. 

Conclusion 

The  opisthobranch  and  pulmonate  gastropods  consti- 
tute large  and  successful  taxa.  In  the  picture  drawn  here, 
selection  during  evolution  has  tightly  interwoven  neuronal 
giantism  in  the  CNS  with  the  physiology  and  behavior  of 
the  animal.  A  number  of  testable  hypotheses  have  been 
proposed,  and  each  can  be  verified  or  falsified  by  more 
detailed,  quantitative  observations.  The  hoped-for  result, 
a  more  complete  resolution  of  the  adaptive  significance 
of  neuronal  giantism,  may  one  day  make  a  useful  con- 
tribution to  our  understanding  of  how  the  nervous  system 
has  evolved  in  tandem  with  behavior. 

Acknowledgment 

The  observations  leading  to  this  paper  were  made  while 
the  author  was  supported  by  NSF  grant  BNS  86-038 16. 

Literature  Cited 

Berry,  M.  S.,  and  V.  W.  Pentreath.  1976.     Properties  of  a  symmetric 

pair  of  serotonin-containing  neurones  in  the  cerebral  ganglia  ofP/iui- 

orbis.  J.  Exp.  Biol.  80:  1 19-135. 
Blackshaw,  S.  E.,  and  D.  A.  Dorset!.  1976.     Behavioural  correlates  of 

activity  in  the  giant  cerebral  neurons  of  Archidoris.  Proc.  R.  Soc. 

Land.  (Biol.)  192:  393-419. 
KMI  hln 1 1/.  R.  1863.     Bemerkungen  uber  den  histologischen  Bau  des 

Zentralnervensystems  der  Susswassermollusken.  Arch.  Anal.  Physio/. 

LPZ  1863:  234-264. 
Bulloch,  A.  G.  M.,  and  D.  A.  Dorsett.  1979.     The  integration  of  the 

patterned  output  of  buccal  motorneurones  during  feeding  in  Tritonia 

homhergi.  J.  Exp.  Biol.  79:  491-508. 
Bullock,  T.  H.  1965.     The  Mollusca.  Pp.  1273-1515  in  Structure  and 

Function  in  the  Nervous  Systems  of  Invertebrates,  \.  2.,  T.  H.  Bullock 

and  G.  A.  Horridge.  eds.  Freeman  Press.  San  Francisco. 
Byrne,  J.  H.,  V.  Castellucci,  and  E.  R.  Kandel.  1974.     Receptive  fields 

and  response  properties  of  mechanoreceptor  neurons  innervating  si- 


phon skin  and  mantle  shelf  in  Aplysia  J  Neurophysiol.  37:  1041- 
1064. 

Cavalier-Smith,  T.  1978.  Nuclear  volume  control  by  nucleoskeletal 
DNA,  selection  for  cell  volume  and  cell  growth  rate,  and  the  solution 
of  the  DNA  C-value  paradox.  J  Cell  Sci.  34:  247-279. 

Chase,  R.,  and  B.  Tolloczko.  1987.  Evidence  for  differential  DNA  en- 
doreplication  during  the  development  of  a  molluscan  brain.  /  Neti- 
rohiol.  18:  395-406. 

Chase,  R.,  and  B.  Tolloczko.  1989.  Interganglionic  dendrites  constitute 
an  output  pathway  from  the  procerebrum  of  the  snail  Achalina  fulica. 
J  Comp  Neural.  283:  143-152. 

Cobbs,  J.  S.,  and  H.  M.  Pinsker.  1979.  //;  vivo  responses  of  paired 
giant  mechanoreceptor  neurons  in  Aply.iia  abdominal  ganglion.  / 
Nftirohiol.  9:  121-141. 

Coggeshall,  R.  E.  1967.  A  light  and  electron  microscope  study  of  the 
abdominal  ganglion  of  Aplysia  califomica.  J  Neurophysiol.  30:  1263- 
12X7. 

Coggeshall,  R.  E.,  B.  A.  Yaksta,  and  F.  J.  Swartz.  1970.  A  cytopho- 
tometric  analysis  of  the  DNA  in  the  nucleus  of  the  giant  cell,  R-2. 
in  Aplysia.  Chromosoma  32:  205-212. 

Dickinson,  P.  S.  1979.  Homologous  neurons  control  movements  of 
diverse  gill  types  in  nudibranch  molluscs.  /  Comp.  Physiol.  131: 
277-283. 

Dickinson,  P.  S.  1980.  Gill  control  in  the  notaspidean  Pleurobranchaea 
and  possible  homologies  with  nudibranchs.  /  Comp.  Physiol.  139: 
1  1-16. 

Dorsett,  D.  A.  1986.  Brains  to  cells:  the  neuroanatomy  of  selected  gas- 
tropod species.  Pp.  101-187  in  The  Mollusca.  K.  M.  Wilbur,  ed.,  v. 
9,  Neufobiology  ami  Behavior,  part ..  A.  O.  D.  Willows,  ed..  Academic 
Press.  New  York. 

Dorsetl,  D.  A.,  and  J.  N.  Sigger.  1981.  Sensory  fields  and  properties 
of  the  oesophageal  propnoceptors  in  the  mollusc,  Pliiline.  J  Exp. 
Biol  94:  77-93. 

Eaton,  C.  M.  1972.  The  reproductive  and  feeding  biology  of  the  pro- 
sobranch  gastropod  Fusitriton  oregonensis  (Redfield)  (Fam.  Cyma- 
tiidae).  University  of  Washington  M.S.  thesis.  40  pp. 

Elliot,  C.  J.  H.,  and  P.  R.  Benjamin.  1985a.  Interactions  of  pattern- 
generating  interneurons  controlling  feeding  in  Lymnaea  stagnalis. 
J  Neurophysiol.  54:  1396-141  I. 

Elliot,  C.  J.  H.,  and  P.  R.  Benjamin.  1985b.  Interactions  of  the  slow 
oscillator  interneuron  with  feeding  pattern-generating  interneurons 
in  Lymnaea  stagnalis.  J  Neurophysiol.  54:  1412-1421. 

Frazier,  W.  T.,  E.  R.  Kandel,  I.  Kupfermann,  R.  VVaziri,  and  R.  E.  Cog- 
geshall. 1967.  Morphological  and  functional  properties  of  identified 
neurons  in  the  abdominal  ganglion  of  Aplysia  califomica.  J.  Neu- 
rophysiol. 30:  1288-1351. 

Gelperin,  A.,  and  D.  \V.  Tank.  1990.  Odour-modulated  collective  net- 
work oscillations  of  olfactory  interneurons  in  a  terrestrial  mollusc. 
Nature  345:  437-440. 

Getting,  P.  A.  1977.  Afferent  neurons  mediating  escape  swimming  of 
the  marine  mollusc  Tritonia.  J.  Comp.  Physiol.  110:  271-286. 

Gillette,  R.,  and  W.  J.  Davis.  1977.  The  role  of  the  metacerebral  giant 
neurone  in  the  feeding  behaviour  of  Pleurobranchaea.  J  Comp. 
Physiol  116:  129-159. 

Gillette,  R.,  M.  U.  Gillette,  and  W.  J.  Davis.  1980.  Action  potential 
broadening  and  endogenously  sustained  bursting  are  substrates  of 
command  ability  in  a  feeding  neuron  of  Pleurobranchaea.  J.  Neu- 
rophysiol. 43:  669-685. 

Gilly,  W.  F.,  and  T.  Brismar.  1989.  Properties  of  appropriately  and 
inappropriately  expressed  sodium  channels  in  squid  giant  axon  and 
its  somata.  /  Neurosci.  9:  1362-1374. 

Gorman,  A.  L.  F.,  and  M.  Mirolli.  1972.  The  passive  electrical  properties 
of  the  membrane  of  a  molluscan  neurone.  J.  Physiol.  (Loud.)  227: 
35-49. 


240 


R.  GILLETTE 


Granzow,  B.,  and  S.  B.  Kater.  1977.     Identified  higher-order  neurones 

controlling  the  feeding  motor  program  ofHelisoma.  Neuroscience  2: 

1049-1063. 
Graubard,  K.  1975.     Voltage  attenuation  within  Ap/ysia  neurons:  the 

effect  of  branching  pattern.  Brain  Rex  88:  325-332. 
Hughes,  G.  M.,  and  L.  Tauc.  1963.     An  electrophysiological  study  of 

the  anatomical  relations  of  two  giant  nerve  cells  in  Ap/ysia  depilans. 

J.  Exp.  Biol.  40:  469-486. 
Jacklet,  J.  \V.  1969.     Circadian  rhythm  of  optic  nerve  impulses  recorded 

in  darkness  from  isolated  eye  of  Aplysia.  Science  164:  562-563. 
Lasek,  R.  J.,  and  W.  J.  Dower.  1971.     Aplysia  californica:  analysis  of 

nuclear  DNA  in  individual  nuclei  of  giant  neurons.  Science  172: 

278-280. 
Lombardo,  F.,  O.  Sonelti,  and  K.  Baraldi.  1980.     Differential  staining 

and  fluorescence  of  chromatin  in  populations  of  neuronal  nuclei  from 

Planorbis.  Nucleus  23:  30-36. 
Masinovsky,  B.,  and  P.  V..  Lloyd.  1985.     Morphology  of  two  pairs  of 

identified  peptidergic  neurons  in  the  buccal  ganglia  of  the  mollusc 

Tnloma  diomedea.  J.  Neurobiol  16:  27-39. 
Mellon,  DeF.,  Jr.,  J.  A.  Wilson,  and  C.  E.  Phillips.  1981 .     Modification 

of  motor  neuron  size  and  position  in  the  central  nervous  system  ot 

adult  snapping  shrimps.  Brain  Res  223:  134-140. 
Mittenlhal,  J.  E.,  and  J.  J.  Wine.  1978.     Segmental  homology  and 

variation  in  flexor  motoneurons  of  the  crayfish  abdomen.  /  Comp. 

Neural.  Ill:  311-334. 
Mirsky,  A.  E.,  and  S.  Osawa.  1961.     The  mterphase  nucleus.  Pp.  677 

770  in  The  Cell.  3.  Brachet  and  A.  E.  Mirsky.  eds.  Academic  Press. 

New  York. 
Mpitsos,  G.  J.,  and  K.  Lukowiak.  1986.     Learning  in  gastropod  molluscs. 

Pp.  95-267  in  The  Mollusca.  K.  M.  Wilbur,  ed.,  v.  8,  Neurobiology 

and  Behavior,  part  1.  A.  O.  D.  Willows,  ed..  Academic  Press,  New 

York. 


Rayport,  S.  G.,  R.  T.  Ambron,  and  J.  Babiarz.  1983.  Identified  cholin- 
ergic  neurons  R2  and  LP1,  control  mucus  release  in  Aplysia  J  Neu- 
mpliysiol  49:  864-876. 

Robertson,  J.  D.,  R.  Gillette,  P.  Lee,  S.  Meadows,  and  J.  Zitz.  1990. 

CNS  neuronal  somata  of  octopus  are  mexcitable  and  label  retrogradely 
with  carbocyanine  dyes.  Sot:  Neurosd.  Abs.  16:  249.7. 

Senseman,  D.,  and  A.  Gelperin.  1973.  Comparative  aspects  of  the 
morphology  and  physiology  of  a  single  identifiable  neurone  in  Helix 
aspersa,  Limax  maxunus.  and  Ariolimax  californica.  Malacol.  Rev 
1:  51-52. 

Siegler,  M.  V.  S.  1977.  Motor  neurone  coordination  and  sensory 
modulation  in  the  feeding  system  of  the  mollusc  Pleurobranchaea 
californica  J  Exp.  Biol.  71:  27-48. 

Spray,  D.  C.,  M.  E.  Spira,  and  M.  V.  L.  Bennett.  1980a.  Peripheral 
fields  and  branching  patterns  of  buccal  mechanosensory  neurons  n 
the  opisthobranch  mollusc  Naranax  inermis.  Brain  Res.  182:  253- 
270. 

Spray,  D.  C,  M.  E.  Spira,  and  M.  V.  L.  Bennett.  1980b.  Synaptic 
connections  of  buccal  mechanosensory  neurons  in  the  opisthobranch 
mollusc  .\avanax  inermis.  Brain  Res.  182:  271-286. 

Spira,  M.  E.,  and  M.  V.  L.  Bennett.  1972.  Synaptic  control  of  electrotonic 
coupling  between  neurons.  Brain  Res.  37:  294-300. 

Weiss,  K.  R.,  and  I.  Kupfermann.  1976.  Homology  of  the  giant  seroton- 
ergic  neurones  (metacerebral  cells)  in  Aplysia  and  pulmonate  mol- 
luscs. Brain  Res  117:  33-49. 

Wells,  M.  J.  1978.  Octopus:  Physiology  and  Behaviour  of  an  Advanced 
Invertebrate.  Chapman  and  Hall,  London. 

Williamson,  R.,  and  B.  U.  Budelmann.  1991.  Convergent  inputs  to 
octopus  oculomotor  neurones  demonstrated  in  a  brain  slice  prepa- 
ration. Neurosci.  Lett.  121:  215-218. 

Young,  J.  Z.  1971.  The  Anatomy  of  the  Nervous  System  (/Octopus 
vulgaris.  Oxford  University  Press,  London. 


Reference:  Butt  Bull  180:  241-251.  (April.  1991) 


A  Functional,  Cellular,  and  Evolutionary  Model  of 
Nociceptive  Plasticity  in  Aplysia 


EDGAR  T.  WALTERS 

Department  of  Physiology  and  Cell  Biology,  University  of  Texas 
Medical  School  at  Houston.  Houston.  Texas  7722? 


Abstract.  Nociceptive  plasticity  is  defined  as  behavioral 
and  cellular  modification  produced  by  activation  of  no- 
ciceptors.  A  brief  survey  of  nociceptive  plasticity  in  Aplysia 
reveals  a  puzzling  mixture  of  behavioral  modifications  of 
opposite  sign  and  widely  varying  durations.  These  include 
general  sensitization.  site-specific  sensitization.  response- 
specific  facilitation,  and  inhibition  of  defensive  responses. 
This  behavioral  complexity  is  more  than  matched  by  the 
complexity  of  cellular  correlates  reported  for  the  behav- 
ioral modifications.  A  functional  model  is  proposed  link- 
ing complex  patterns  of  behavioral  and  neural  plasticity 
in  Aplysia  to  potentially  general  principles  of  nociceptive 
function.  This  model  is  centered  around  three  overlapping 
but  functionally  distinct  phases:  injury  detection,  escape, 
and  recuperation.  A  hypothesis  about  the  early  origin  of 
nociceptive  plasticity  in  primitive  mechanosensory  neu- 
rons is  then  developed,  based  on  similarities  in  the  or- 
ganization and  modinability  of  nociceptive  systems  in 
evolutionarily  divergent  groups  (primarily  mollusks  and 
mammals)  and  on  inferences  about  the  early  adaptiveness 
of  postinjury  behavioral  plasticity.  Preliminary  evidence 
suggests  that  aspects  of  nociceptive  plasticity,  and  perhaps 
other  forms  of  memory,  may  have  been  derived  from  cel- 
lular repair  and  signal  compensation  mechanisms. 

Introduction 

Neuronal  mechanisms  controlling  withdrawal  re- 
sponses in  the  opisthobranch  mollusc,  Aplysia  californica, 
have  been  studied  extensively  by  neurobiologists  for  over 
two  decades.  Investigators  have  been  attracted  to  the  gill. 


siphon,  and  tail  withdrawal  responses  of  Aplysia,  in  part 
because  the  CNS  can  be  readily  analyzed  in  this  animal, 
but  largely  because  these  responses  and  their  underlying 
neurophysiology  display  a  remarkable  degree  of  modin- 
ability. The  rare  opportunity,  provided  by  Aplysia  and 
several  other  gastropod  mollusks,  to  link  behavioral  and 
cellular  alterations  has  been  used  to  advantage  by  several 
laboratories  and  has  led  to  the  discovery  of  various  mech- 
anisms contributing  to  learning  and  memory  in  this  spe- 
cies. 

Although  we  presume  that  some  mechanisms  from 
gastropod  "model  systems"  are  general,  the  possibility  that 
a  given  mechanism  will  be  common  to  groups  as  evolu- 
tionarily divergent  as  are  the  mollusks  and  mammals  re- 
quires serious  scrutiny.  As  a  first  step  in  examining  the 
potential  generality  of  mechanisms  contributing  to  learn- 
ing and  memory  in  Aplysia,  I  will  discuss  some  nociceptive 
functions  of  these  mechanisms,  propose  a  three-phase 
model  of  nociceptive  plasticity,  and  consider  the  possi- 
bility that  mechanisms  of  nociceptive  plasticity  evolved 
in  primitive  mechanosensory  neurons  and  have  been 
conserved  in  diverse  phyla.  Central  to  this  discussion  is 
that  most  cellular  mechanisms  of  behavioral  modification 
revealed  to  date  in  Aplysia  have  been  produced  either  by 
noxious  stimulation,  or  by  manipulations  that  mimic  ef- 
fects of  noxious  stimulation.  The  behavioral  and  cellular 
modifications  are  thus  examples  of  nociceptive  plasticity, 
which  I  define  as  modifications  induced  by  the  activation 
of  nociceptors.  Nociceptors  are  defined  as  sensory  neurons 
that  are  activated  maximally  by  stimuli  that,  if  sufficiently 
prolonged,  cause  tissue  damage  (Sherrington,  1906). 


Received  14  August  1990;  accepted  22  January  1991. 

Abbreviations:  Activity-dependent  extrinsic  modulation,  ADEM; 
central  nervous  system,  CNS;  Excitatory  postsynaptic  potential.  EPSP; 
Phe-Met-Arg-Phe-NH;,  FMRFamide;  serotonin,  5-HT. 


Forms  of  Nociceptive  Plasticity 

Noxious  stimuli,  such  as  strong  pinch  or  shock,  were 
at  first  assumed  to  have  only  two  major  effects  on  Aplysia: 


241 


242 


E.  T.  WALTERS 


to  trigger  vigorous  defensive  responses,  and  to  cause  gen- 
eral sensitization  of  the  animal  for  several  minutes.  The 
term  "sensitization"  has  been  used  independently  by  psy- 
chologists and  physiologists  to  describe  an  increase  in 
sensitivity  or  magnitude  of.  respectively,  a  behavioral  or 
physiological  response.  I  define  nociceptive  sensitization 
as  sensitization  produced  by  noxious  stimulation,  where 
sensitization  is  defined  physiologically  as  an  increase  in 
sensitivity  or  responsiveness  of  the  organism  to  a  constant 
test  stimulus.  Such  hypersensitivity  after  noxious  stimu- 
lation need  not  be  expressed  as  overt  behavior,  but  is  often 
expressed  as  a  decrease  in  threshold  and  an  increase  in 
the  magnitude  of  defensive  responses  evoked  by  a  test 
stimulus.  By  this  definition,  sensitization  may  also  be  ex- 
pressed as  inhibition  of  ongoing  behavior  (usually  non- 
defensive)  by  the  test  stimulus.  Nociceptive  sensitization 
can  be  general  (expressed  by  changes  in  response  to  a 
broad  range  of  test  stimuli  and  stimulation  sites)  or,  as 
discussed  below,  specific  to  a  warning  signal  or  to  a  re- 
stricted site  on  the  body.  The  apparent  function  of  general 
nociceptive  sensitization  is  to  prime  the  animal  for  con- 
tinued defense,  so  that  it  responds  rapidly  and  energeti- 
cally to  a  wide  range  of  stimuli  that  might  presage  an 
attack. 

The  first  clue  that  nociceptive  plasticity  involves  more 
than  a  brief,  general  sensitization  came  from  the  obser- 
vation that  repeated  application  of  noxious  stimuli  over 
hours  or  days  causes  general  sensitization  of  siphon  with- 
drawal that  can  last  for  weeks  (Pinsker  et  al..  1973;  Frost 
el  al.,  1985).  It  was  then  discovered  that  sensitization  can 
be  conditioned  to  a  warning  signal;  a  variety  of  defensive 
responses  are  selectively  facilitated  by  a  chemosensory  cue 
(e.g.,  shrimp  extract)  if  it  is  repeatedly  paired  with  noxious 
shock  (Walters  et  al.,  1981;  Colwill  et  al..  1988).  Further 
links  between  sensitization  and  associative  processes  were 
indicated  by  behavioral  data  (Carew  el  al.,  1981,  1983; 
Hawkins  et  al.,  1983)  and  neuronal  data  (see  next  section), 
suggesting  considerable  overlap  of  sensitization  and  pu- 
tative classical  conditioning  mechanisms  within  individual 
sensory  neurons. 

The  next  discovery,  site-specific  sensitization,  is  crucial 
to  the  functional  and  evolutionary  arguments  of  this  pa- 
per. Noxious  stimulation  enhances  siphon  and  tail  with- 
drawal test  responses;  but  responses  evoked  by  test  stimuli 
applied  near  the  site  of  noxious  stimulation  are  more  dra- 
matic than  those  evoked  by  test  stimuli  applied  at  other 
sites  on  the  body  (Walters,  1987a).  The  site-specific  be- 
havioral plasticity  is  particularly  potent;  a  single  45  s  nox- 
ious stimulation  sequence  that  is  insufficient  to  cause  long- 
term  general  sensitization  produces  site-specific  sensiti- 
zation lasting  a  week  or  more  (Fig.  1 ). 

The  complexity  of  nociceptive  plasticity  was  under- 
scored recently  when  several  groups  found  that  noxious 
stimulation  can  inhibit,  as  well  as  enhance,  defensive  re- 


A. 


Siphon 


Mantle  Cavity 

\ 


Rhinophores 


Tail 


Foot 


Tentacles 


B. 

80 
60 
40 
20 


Training 


/   \  1 

Pre      2  hr 


Days 


Figure  1.  Site-specific  sensitization  following  noxious  tail  stimulation 
in  freely  moving  Aplyxia.  (A)  Diagram  of  unrestrained  animal  used  for 
testing  and  training.  Before  and  after  site-specific  sensitization  training, 
weak  test  stimuli  were  applied  with  a  hand-held  electrode  to  a  site  on 
each  side  of  the  tail  that  had  been  marked  with  a  suture  (not  shown). 
Training  consisted  of  a  45  s  sequence  of  strong  shocks  to  one  of  the  test 
sites.  During  each  test,  the  duration  of  siphon  withdrawal  was  timed, 
and  the  magnitude  of  tail  withdrawal  was  estimated.  (B)  Site-specific 
sensitization  of  siphon  withdrawal.  Siphon  withdrawal  was  significantly 
greater  when  tested  at  the  trained  site  than  the  contralateral  control  site. 
Similar  differences  were  seen  in  tail  withdrawal  (not  shown)  and  when 
other  parts  of  the  body  were  trained  and  tested  (Walters, 


sponses  (Krontiris-Litowitz  et  a!..  1987;  Mackey  et  al.. 
1987;  Marcus  et  al.,  1988).  The  most  complete  behavioral 
study  of  nociceptive  inhibition  was  reported  by  Marcus 
et  al.  (1988),  who  showed  that  inhibition  of  siphon  with- 
drawal occurs  following  noxious  but  not  innocuous  stim- 
uli, and  that  net  inhibition  has  a  brief  duration. 

These  various  forms  of  nociceptive  plasticity  differ  in 
the  sign,  duration,  and  stimulus  specificity  of  behavioral 
modulation.  Yet  another  dimension  of  plasticity  was  re- 
vealed by  the  discovery  that  particular  siphon  responses 
are  modulated  selectively  by  noxious  stimulation  of  dif- 


NOCICEPTIVE  PLASTICITY  IN  APLYSIA 


243 


Intact  Animal 


Reduced  Preparation 


A. 


Relaxed 


siphon 


mantle 

shell      ink  gland 


parapodium 


gill 


B. 


Flaring,  Tail-type  Response 


posterior  stimulation 


C. 


Constricting,  Head-type  Response 


anterior  stimulation 


D. 


Response  Transformation 


Anterior  US 


Posterior  US  10  sec 

Figure  2.  Transformation  of  siphon  responses  following  noxious 
stimulation.  The  left  column  shows  a  cutaway  view  of  the  siphon  and 
mantle  organs  in  the  intact  animal  (compare  Fig.  1  A).  The  right  column 
shows  the  mantle  organs  and  CNS  in  a  reduced  preparation.  The  photocell 
monitors  the  breadth  but  not  the  length  of  the  siphon.  (A)  Relaxed  siphon. 
Weak  test  stimuli  were  applied  to  a  midbody  nerve  at  1  min  intervals. 
A  noxious  unconditioned  stimulus  (US),  a  1 5  s  sequence  of  strong  shock, 
was  delivered  to  either  a  tentacle  nerve  or  a  tail  nerve.  (B)  Flanng  response, 
typical  of  posterior  stimulation.  The  photocell  shows  a  negative  deflection. 
(C)  Constricting  response  typical  of  anterior  stimulation.  The  photocell 
shows  a  positive  deflection.  (D)  Examples  of  transformed  responses. 
Top— flaring  responses  are  converted  to  constricting  responses  after 
noxious  anterior  stimulation.  Bottom— constricting  responses  are  con- 
verted to  flaring  responses  after  noxious  posterior  stimulation  (Erickson 
and  Walters,  1988). 


ferent  regions  of  the  body.  This  response-specific  noci- 
ceptive  plasticity  is  expressed  most  clearly  when  noxious 
stimulation  causes  the  animal  to  respond  to  a  test  stimulus 
with  a  qualitatively  different  response  than  it  did  before 
noxious  stimulation  (Erickson  and  Walters,  1988).  Figure 
2  shows  examples  of  siphon  responses  being  transformed 
into  opposite  responses  following  intense  stimulation  of 
nerves  from  the  head  or  tail.  Like  sensitization,  response 
transformation  can  be  enhanced  by  associative  training. 
The  incidence  and  degree  of  transformation  of  motor  re- 
sponses to  particular  test  stimuli  are  preferentially  in- 
creased if  the  test  stimulus  is  repeatedly  paired  with  a 
noxious  stimulus  (Walters,  1989;  Hawkins  el  til..  1989). 


Mechanisms  of  Nociceptive  Plasticity 

Mechanisms  of  general  sensitization  in  Aplysia  have 
received  detailed  analysis.  Here  I  briefly  describe  selected 
aspects  of  cellular  mechanisms,  focusing  on  those  that 
have  been  closely  linked  to  changes  in  defensive  behavior. 
For  reviews  of  subcellular  mechanisms  of  sensitization. 
see  Kandel  and  Schwartz  (1982)  and  Byrne  (1987). 

In  principle,  sensitization  might  involve  alterations  in 
any  of  various  classes  of  neurons  known  to  contribute  to 
defensive  behavior  in  Aplysia  (Fig.  3).  Although  some  in- 
terneurons  and  motor  neurons  show  alterations  during 
general  sensitization  (e.g..  Frost  el  til..  1988),  analysis  has 
centered  on  mechanosensory  neurons:  the  LE  cluster, 
which  innervates  the  siphon  (Byrne  et  a/..  1974);  and  the 
VC  clusters,  which  innervate  most  of  the  rest  of  the  body 
(Walters  et  al..  1 983a).  No  major  differences  between  these 
sensory  clusters  have  been  described  in  their  response 
properties  or  plasticity.  Cells  in  both  clusters  show  a  wide 
dynamic  range,  responding  weakly  to  stimuli  of  moderate 
intensity  and  more  strongly  as  stimulus  intensity  is  in- 


General  Neural  Organization 
Underlying  Nociceptive  Behavior  in  Aplysia 


Figure  3.  General  pattern  of  neural  organization  controlling  noci- 
ceptive  behavior  in  Aplysiu.  Each  indicated  population  of  cells  may  in- 
clude hundreds  of  neurons  distributed  throughout  the  nervous  system. 
Wide-dynamic  range  nociceptive  sensory  neurons  (S)  innervate  the  entire 
body  surface.  Each  cell  connects  to  peripheral  motor  neurons  (P),  to 
central  motor  neurons  (M)  innervating  the  same  region,  and  to  inhibitory, 
excitatory,  and  facilitatory  interneurons.  Sensory  neurons  also  make 
connections  (largely  polysynaptic)  to  complex  pattern  generating  networks 
responsible  for  rhythmic  defensive  behaviors  such  as  mantle  pumping 
(used  to  eject  ink  and  to  increase  respiration  and  blood  circulation)  and 
escape  locomotion.  Relatively  little  is  known  about  interconnections 
among  the  various  types  of  interneurons.  A  further  complication  is  that 
some  interneurons  are  multifunctional  U'.#.,  having  both  excitatory  and 
facilitatory  effects  on  the  same  follower  neuron).  Based  on  data  from 
Bailey  et  al.  (1979),  Byrne  (1980,  1983),  Hawkins  et  al.  (1981),  Frost  et 
al.  (1988),  and  Hickie  and  Walters  (unpub.  obs.). 


244 


E.  T.  WALTERS 


creased  (Byrne  el  a/..  1978;  Walters  el  al..  1983a).  Both 
clusters  have  nociceptive  functions,  because  they  respond 
maximally  to  noxious  pinching  stimuli  (Walters  el  at.. 
1983a:  Walters  and  Clatworthy,  unpub.  obs.).  and  they 
are  therefore  indicated,  in  Figure  3,  within  the  circle  la- 
beled "nociceptors". 

Sensory  neurons  in  the  LE  cluster  (Bailey  el  al..  1979), 
and  probably  in  other  central  nociceptive  clusters  (e.g., 
Walters,  1987b),  make  some  synaptic  connections  to  pe- 
ripheral motor  neurons  (Fig.  3).  But  with  few  exceptions 
(see  Clark  and  Kandel,  1984),  analysis  of  synaptic  plas- 
ticity in  these  sensory  populations  has  focused  on  their 
strong  monosynaptic  connections  to  identified  motor 
neurons  within  the  CNS.  Because  of  these  connections 
and  others  to  excitatory,  facilitatory.  and  inhibitory  in- 
terneurons  involved  in  defensive  responses  (Fig.  3), 
changes  in  the  signalling  properties  of  LE  and  VC  sensory 
neurons  should  have  potent  effects  on  behavioral  re- 
sponses elicited  by  moderate  to  strong  cutaneous  stimuli. 
Short-term  behavioral  sensitization  is  correlated  with 
general  facilitation  of  synapses  from  sensory  neurons  to 
motor  and  interneurons  (Kandel  and  Schwartz,  1982; 
Walters  el  al..  1983b)  and  with  increased  excitability  of 
peripheral  branches  of  the  sensory  neuron  (Clatworthy 
and  Walters,  1990).  The  presynaptic  facilitation  is  me- 
diated, at  least  in  part,  by  5-HT  (Glanzman  et  al..  1989), 
which  can  also  enhance  excitability  of  the  central  and 
peripheral  parts  of  the  sensory  neuron  (Walters  et  al., 
1983b;  Klein  et  al..  1986;  Billy  and  Walters,  1989b).  Many 
of  the  effects  of  5-HT  are  mediated  by  cyclic  AMP-de- 
pendent  protein  kinase  (Kandel  and  Schwartz,  1982),  and 
some  are  mediated  by  protein  kinase  C  (Braha  el  a/.. 
1990).  The  most  notable  effects  involve  the  depression  of 
K+  conductances  (Klein  et  ul.,  1982;  Baxter  and  Byrne, 
1989;  Walsh  and  Byrne,  1989).  which  increase  transmitter 
release  and  excitability  by  broadening  spikes  and  decreas- 
ing spike  accomodation  (Kandel  and  Schwartz,  1982; 
Walters  et  al.,  1983b;  Klein  et  al..  1986).  Noxious  stim- 
ulation also  appears  to  enhance  a  Ca2+  conductance  (Ed- 
monds et  al..  1990). 

The  expression  of  long-term  sensitization  in  sensory 
neurons  involves  some  of  the  same  mechanisms  as  short- 
term  sensitization:  depressed  K '  conductances,  increased 
transmitter  release,  and  increased  excitability  (Frost  et  al., 
1985;  Scholz  and  Byrne,  1987;  Walters,  1987b).  Specific 
morphological  changes  also  occur  in  the  sensory  neuron, 
including  the  growth  of  new  synaptic  varicosities  and  ac- 
tive zones  within  the  CNS  (Bailey  and  Chen,  1983,  1988; 
Nazif  et  al..  1989),  and  possibly  the  growth  of  peripheral 
processes  that  expand  the  size  of  the  receptive  field  (Billy 
and  Walters,  1989a).  Considerable  effort  is  being  made 
to  identify  molecular  mechanisms  involved  in  inducing 
and  maintaining  long-term  changes  in  these  sensory  neu- 
rons (e.g.,  Barzilai  et  al.,  1989:  Eskin  el  al.,  1989). 


Associative  enhancement  of  withdrawal  responses  to 
mechanosensory  cues  and  site-specific  sensitization  ap- 
pears to  involve  the  same  basic  mechanism:  activity-de- 
pendent enhancement  of  the  mechanisms  of  general  sen- 
sitization. as  described  above  (Walters  and  Byrne,  1983a; 
Hawkins  et  al..  1983:  Walters.  1987b).  Figure  4  illustrates 
two  of  the  sensory  neuron  alterations  contributing  to  long- 
term  site-specific  sensitization:  synaptic  facilitation  and 
increased  soma  excitability;  the  latter  is  expressed  dra- 
matically as  a  prolonged  afterdischarge  to  brief  depolar- 
ization. During  the  induction  of  site-specific  sensitization. 
the  sensory  neuron  is  activated  by  the  noxious  stimulus. 
In  associative  conditioning,  the  sensory  neuron  is  activated 
by  a  cue  presented  immediately  before  the  noxious  stim- 
ulus. In  both  cases,  the  activity  enhances  the  effects  of 
extrinsic  chemical  modulators  (e.g.,  5-HT)  on  the  sensory 
neuron,  and  thus  this  general  class  of  plasticity  is  termed 
activity-dependent  extrinsic  modulation  (ADEM).  Acti- 
vation of  sensory  neurons  opens  Ca:+  channels  (Walters 
and  Byrne,  1983b;  Edmonds  et  al.,  1990).  The  resulting 
Ca+  influx  enhances  adenylate  cyclase  activity,  increasing 
the  rate  of  cyclic  AMP  synthesis,  and  thus  amplifying  the 
degree  and  duration  of  plasticity  induced  by  neuromodu- 
lators  released  during  noxious  stimulation  (Abrams  and 
Kandel.  1988).  Ca:+  might  also  enhance  plasticity  in  other 
ways;  for  example,  Ca24 -dependent  kinases  may  directly 
phosphorylate  transcription  factors  (cf.  Dash  et  a/..  1990). 

The  cellular  mechanisms  of  nociceptive  inhibition  have 
also  been  studied.  Mackey  et  al.  (1987)  reported  that  tail 
shock  causes  presynaptic  inhibition  and  spike  narrowing 
in  siphon  sensory  neurons,  and  that  these  effects  are  partly 
mediated  by  an  identified  interneuron  containing  the 
neuropeptide  FMRFamide.  Application  of  FMRFamide 
to  the  sensory  cell  soma  and  synaptic  region  in  the  CNS 
causes  hyperpolarization,  spike  narrowing,  and  presyn- 


A    Control  side         B  Trained  side 


Sensory 
Neuron 


5mV 
30mV 


Figure  4.  Example  of  synaptic  facilitation  and  afterdischarge  in  a 
sensory  neuron  after  site-specific  sensitization.  The  intact  animal  was 
trained  with  strong  noxious  shock  delivered  to  one  side  of  the  body.  One 
day  later,  the  CNS  was  removed  and  sensory  and  motor  neurons  inner- 
vating the  trained  side  and  corresponding  regions  on  the  contralateral 
side  were  examined.  (A)  Typical  connection  between  a  tail  sensory  neuron 
and  tail  motor  neuron  innervating  the  untrained  side  of  the  tail.  The 
connection  was  tested  by  activating  the  sensory  neuron  with  a  10  ms 
depolarizing  pulse  injected  into  the  soma.  (B)  The  same  test  procedure 
on  the  trained  side  elicits  a  larger  synaptic  potential  and  an  afterdischarge 
of  20  spikes  (Walters,  1987b). 


NOCICEPTIVE  PLASTICITY  IN  APLYSIA 


245 


Table  I 

I'lircc-i>lhi\c  model  of  the  functions  and  general  meehani\ni\  «l  nociceptive  pluMiaiy  in  Aplysia 


Phase  1  .  Injury  detection 

Phase  2.  Escape 

Phase  3.  Recuperation 

Period: 

Functions: 

0.1  s-10  mm 
•  Severity  appraisal 

1  s-30  min 
•  Flight 

10  min-1  month 
•  General  inactivity 

Mechanisms: 


>  Localization 

•  Compensation  for  destruction 

of  nociceptive  channels 

•  Defensive  response  triggering 

>  Anticipation 

>  Nociceptor  activation 

a.  Frequency  code 

b.  Wide  dynamic  range 

>  Activation  of  defensive  circuits 

(Somatotopic  organization) 

>  Nociceptor  facilitation 
a.  Afterdischarge 

h.  PTP 

c.  HSF 

d.  Hyperexcitability 

e.  ADEM 

•  Motor  facilitation 


1  Inhibition  of  competing 
responses 


•  Activity  in  circuits  generating 

escape  behavior 

•  Nociceptor  inhibition 

a.  Presynaptic  inhibition 

(neuromodulation) 

b.  Activity-dependent  reduction 

in  excitability 
>  Inhibition  of  motor  and 
interneurons  controlling 
other  behaviors 


(healing) 

•  Defensive  readiness 

(sensitization) 

a.  General 

b.  Wound  specific 

c.  Cue  specific 

•  Inhibition  of  circuits 

controlling  feeding, 
reproduction,  etc. 

•  Nociceptor  facilitation 

a.  ADEM 

b.  Axon  injury  signals 

c.  Lower  threshold 

d.  Less  accomodation 

e.  Afterdischarge 

f.  Synaptic  facilitation 

g.  Sprouting 

•  Motor  facilitation 


The  times  for  each  phase  indicate  the  approximate  beginning  and  end  of  the  phase  relative  to  the  beginning  of  the  noxious  stimulus.  ADEM — 
activity-dependent  extrinsic  modulation;  HSF — heterosynaptic  facilitation;  PTP — posttetanic  potentiation. 


aptic  inhibition  in  sensory  neurons  (Belardetti  ct  til..  1987), 
whereas  peripheral  application  increases  mechanosensory 
threshold  (Billy  and  Walters,  1989b).  Extrapolation  from 
studies  in  other  mollusks  suggests  that  other  neuromodu- 
lators  may  also  contribute  to  nociceptive  inhibition  of 
defensive  responses  in  Aplysia.  For  example,  pharmaco- 
logical evidence  suggests  that  inhibition  of  a  defensive 
response  in  another  gastropod,  the  snail  Cepaea,  may  in- 
volve opiate-like  modulators  (Kavaliers,  1987).  In  Aplysia. 
noxious  stimulation  also  produces  activity-dependent  re- 
duction in  the  excitability  of  sensory  neuron  axons  and 
receptive  fields  (Clatworthy  and  Walters,  1990),  and  can 
sometimes  block  afferent  spikes  (Clatworthy  and  Wal- 
ters, 1989).  The  presynaptic  inhibition  produced  by 
FMRFamide  is  also  activity-dependent  (Small  el  at.. 
1989).  Recently,  Wright  el  al  (1989)  suggested  that  in- 
terneurons may  be  more  important  loci  than  sensory 
neurons  for  inhibition  in  the  siphon  withdrawal  system. 
They  found  that  tail  shock  suppressed  polysynaptic  (in- 
terneuronal)  components  of  a  complex  test  EPSP  in  si- 
phon motor  neurons  under  conditions  in  which  no  in- 
hibition of  the  monosynaptic  EPSP  from  the  sensory  neu- 
rons was  detected. 

Mechanisms  of  response-specific  nociceptive  plasticity 
are  not  yet  known.  However,  tests  of  several  potential 
mechanisms  have  recently  begun  in  identified  neurons 
within  siphon  control  circuits  (Erickson  and  Walters, 


1988;  Frost  et  al.  1988:  Hickie  and  Walters,  1990;  Fang 
and  Clark,  1990). 

A  Functional  Model  of  Nociceptive  Plasticity 

These  findings  show  that  noxious  stimulation  causes 
highly  complex  behavioral  and  neuronal  alterations  in 
Aplvsia.  Two  issues  have  not  been  clear:  the  functional 
significance  of  this  complexity,  and  the  integration  of  ap- 
parently opposing  forms  of  plasticity  to  produce  adaptive 
behavior.  General  similarities  between  the  patterns  of  no- 
ciceptive behavior  observed  in  Aplysia  and  in  other  species 
(primarily  rats  and  humans)  suggest  that  forms  of  noci- 
ceptive plasticity  in  Aplysia  might  represent  common  be- 
havioral adaptations  to  ubiquitous  selection  pressures; 
namely,  escape  from  a  source  of  bodily  injury,  and  op- 
timization of  recuperation.  This  possibility  encouraged 
the  formulation  of  a  model  linking  potentially  general 
principles  of  nociceptive  function  to  patterns  of  behavioral 
and  neural  plasticity  that  have  been  described  in  Aplysia. 
In  a  functional  model  of  pain  and  fear  in  mammals,  Bolles 
and  Fanselow  (1980)  divided  nociceptive  responses  into 
perceptual,  defensive,  and  recuperative  phases.  Somewhat 
similar  phases  can  be  used  in  a  functional  model  to  explain 
much  of  the  complexity  of  behavioral  and  neuronal  plas- 
ticity observed  in  Aplysia  following  noxious  stimulation 
(Table  I).  These  overlapping  phases  of  injury  detection. 


246 


E.  T.  WALTERS 


escape,  and  recuperation  correspond  to  periods  of  im- 
mediate facilitation,  short-term  inhibition,  and  long-term 
facilitation  of  defensive  responses. 

Phase  1 — injury  detection 

How  does  an  animal  know  it  is  injured,  or  about  to  be 
injured?  False  negative  answers  mean  an  animal  will  fail 
to  initiate  escape  and  recuperative  behavior,  jeopardizing 
its  life.  False  positive  answers  also  reduce  biological  fitness 
by  committing  the  animal  unnecessarily  to  energy-con- 
suming escape  behavior,  and  possibly  to  a  long  period  of 
recuperative  behavior  during  which  important  activities 
such  as  reproduction  and  feeding  are  inhibited.  One  way 
for  a  CNS  to  decide  whether  an  injury  has  occurred  is  to 
interpret  any  activity  on  nociceptive  labeled  lines  from 
the  body  as  proof  of  injury.  However,  because  an  animal 
needs  to  match  its  responses  to  the  severity  and  location 
of  its  injuries,  nociceptive  signals  should  also  carry  inten- 
sity and  spatial  information.  In  addition,  if  the  severity 
of  an  injury  is  represented  by  the  number  of  active  no- 
ciceptive fibers  and  by  their  degree  of  activity,  there  should 
be  some  way  of  compensating  for  the  loss  of  signal  strength 
during  and  after  injury  severe  enough  to  destroy  or  damage 
nociceptive  fibers  from  the  injured  region.  Finally,  a  no- 
ciceptive system  would  be  highly  adaptive  if  it  could  rec- 
ognize noxious  stimuli  prior  to  actual  injury. 

These  general  functional  considerations  are  reflected 
in  the  organization  of  the  nociceptive  system  of  Aplysia 
and  in  the  alterations  of  this  system  that  immediately  fol- 
low moderate  intensity  or  noxious  cutaneous  stimulation 
(Table  I).  LE  and  VC  sensory  neurons  trigger  defensive 
withdrawal  responses,  and  the  magnitude  of  the  responses 
thus  evoked  depends  upon  the  number  of  LE  or  VC  neu- 
rons activated,  upon  the  number  and  frequency  of  spikes 
generated,  and  upon  the  amount  of  transmitter  released 
per  spike  ( By  rne  el  al. ,  1978;  Walters  eta  I..  1 9  8  3a;  Walters, 
unpub.  obs.).  Therefore,  the  likelihood  and  severity  of 
body  wall  injury  in  Aplysia  appear  to  be  coded,  at  least 
in  part,  by  the  total  level  of  activity  in  these  nociceptive 
channels  and  by  the  strength  of  nociceptive  connections 
to  interneurons  and  motor  neurons.  As  in  mammals,  the 
relatively  small  size  of  nociceptive  receptive  fields  and  the 
somatotopic  organization  of  sensory  and  motor  pathways 
in  Aplysia  (Walters  el  al.,  1983a)  contribute  to  the  local- 
ization of  noxious  stimuli. 

How  does  this  system  compensate  for  the  destruction 
of  nociceptive  axons  during  severe  injury?  The  VC  and 
LE  sensory  neurons  provide  labeled  lines  to  the  CNS  from 
the  periphery,  but  their  wide  dynamic  range  and  response 
properties  also  make  possible  a  frequency  code  for  the 
severity  of  injury.  A  brief,  punctate,  moderately  intense 
stimulus  to  the  tail,  which  does  not  cause  injury  unless 
greatly  prolonged,  typically  evokes  one  to  five  spikes  in 


each  of  three  to  five  VC  neurons,  the  receptive  fields  of 
which  are  estimated  to  overlap  any  given  point  on  the  tail 
(Walters  el  al.,  1983a;  Billy  and  Walters,  1989a).  The  total 
activity  in  these  sensory  neurons  ( 10-20  spikes  over  about 
0.5  s)  leads  to  a  relatively  brief  withdrawal  of  the  tail  and 
siphon,  and  perhaps  to  escape  locomotion.  A  strong, 
punctate,  pinching  stimulus  of  the  same  duration,  which 
may  cause  some  cutaneous  damage  but  does  not  destroy 
major  axons  of  VC  sensory  neurons,  causes  high  frequency 
activation  of  each  sensory  neuron,  but  the  activation  rarely 
outlasts  a  moderately  noxious  stimulus.  Thus,  a  0.5  s 
stimulus  might  lead  to  a  0.5  s  barrage  of  perhaps  75  spikes 
across  the  same  5  VC  neurons,  leading  to  strong,  long- 
lasting  withdrawal  of  the  tail  and  siphon,  as  well  as  to 
inking  and  vigorous  escape  locomotion.  Finally,  a  severe, 
crushing  stimulus  of  the  same  duration  will  probably  de- 
stroy some  of  the  sensory  axons  innervating  the  region. 
Assuming,  for  illustrative  purposes,  that  axons  from  three 
of  the  five  sensory  neurons  are  destroyed,  how  is  the  CNS 
informed  of  the  severity  of  the  injury?  First,  the  remaining 
fibers  will  fire  at  maximal  frequency  (about  50  Hz)  during 
the  stimulus.  Second,  the  crushed  axons  will  produce  an 
injury  discharge  of  high  frequency  spikes.  Third,  very 
strong  stimuli  produce  an  afterdischarge  in  VC  sensory 
neurons  (see  Fig.  4)  that  can  last  0.1  to  3  s  (Clatworthy 
and  Walters,  1988,  and  unpub.  obs.).  The  afterdischarge 
is  generated,  at  least  in  part,  within  the  CNS  (Clatworthy 
and  Walters,  1988),  raising  the  possibility  that  both  the 
intact  VC  neurons,  and  the  VC  neurons  with  injury-de- 
stroyed axons,  fire  at  high  frequency  during  the  noxious 
stimulus  and  for  1  to  2  s  afterwards.  Thus,  even  with  a 
majority  of  the  sensory  fibers  from  the  injured  region  dis- 
connected from  the  CNS  by  the  injury,  a  barrage  of  100- 
200  high  frequency  sensory  spikes  may  reach  central  syn- 
aptic  terminals  onto  defensive  motor  and  interneurons. 
The  mechanism  of  afterdischarge  is  not  yet  known,  but 
it  appears  to  depend  upon  both  the  initial  spike  activity 
and  the  extracellular  release  of  chemical  modulators,  i.e., 
ADEM  (Clatworthy  and  Walters,  unpub.  obs.).  Finally, 
under  natural  conditions,  more  severe  stimuli  usually  af- 
fect larger  areas  of  body  wall  and  thus  activate  more  no- 
ciceptors. 

How  does  the  system  anticipate  injury  during  moderate 
intensity  cutaneous  stimulation  that  threatens  but  does 
not  immediately  produce  tissue  damage?  Stimuli  suffi- 
ciently intense  to  activate  LE  and  VC  neurons,  but  not 
severe  enough  to  cause  immediate  body  wall  injury,  have 
transient  facilitatory  effects  upon  defensive  responses.  This 
facilitation  involves  brief  (seconds  to  minutes)  heterosy- 
naptic  facilitation  (Carew  el  al.,  1971;  Walters  el  al., 
1983b),  post-tetanic  potentiation  (Walters  and  Byrne, 
1984;  Clark  and  Kandel,  1984),  and  enhanced  peripheral 
excitability  (Clatworthy  and  Walters,  1990)  in  the  sensory 
neurons.  The  facilitation  of  peripheral  excitability,  as  well 


NOCICEPTIVE  PLASTICITY  IN  A/'LYS/A 


247 


as  facilitation  of  synaptic  transmission  (Hawkins  el  ai, 
1983;  Walters  and  Byrne,  1983a),  is  greatest  in  sensory 
neurons  activated  by  the  noxious  stimulus.  As  a  conse- 
quence, continued  or  repeated  application  of  a  moderately 
noxious  stimulus  to  the  same  region  should  cause  in- 
creasing activation  of  the  nociceptors,  increasing  synaptic 
facilitation  (Walters  ct  a/..  1983b),  and  increasingly  effec- 
tive sensory  input  to  the  CNS.  Temporal  summation  of 
excitatory  and  facilitatory  inputs  to  defensive  interneurons 
and  motor  neurons  occurs  (Carew  and  Kandel,  1977; 
Walters,  unpub.  obs.),  facilitating  motor  responsiveness. 
These  effects  also  increase  the  spontaneous  firing  rates  of 
some  motor  neurons,  which  can  lead  to  neuromuscular 
facilitation  (e.g..  Frost  ct  a/..  1988).  All  of  these  sensory 
and  motor  facilitation  mechanisms  produce  "windup"  of 
neural  responses  to  repeated  or  prolonged  stimulation  that 
is  intense  enough  to  be  threatening.  Windup  has  two  con- 
sequences: withdrawal  and  escape  responses  are  triggered 
before  a  prolonged,  moderately  noxious  stimulus  injures 
the  animal;  and  the  animal  is  prepared  to  respond  max- 
imally if  more  severe  stimulation  follows.  Windup  of  re- 
sponses to  noxious  stimuli  in  mammals  appears  to  involve 
some  of  the  same  mechanisms  (Woolf  and  Walters,  1991). 
Brief  habituating  and  inhibitory  effects,  reported  for  rel- 
atively weak  cutaneous  stimuli  in  Aplysia  (e.g..  Kupfer- 
mann  et  ai.  1970;  Mackey  ct  ai.  1987).  should  oppose 
and  delay  these  facilitatory  effects,  reducing  the  chances 
of  overreaction  to  innocuous  stimuli. 

Phase  2 — escape 

When  the  CNS  interprets  a  stimulus  as  injurious  or 
potentially  injurious,  escape  behavior  is  initiated,  and  the 
second  phase  of  nociceptive  plasticity  begins.  It  has  long 
been  observed  that  animals  in  the  act  of  fleeing  or  fighting 
ignore  their  injuries.  Nociceptive  responses  are  inhibited, 
and  this  inhibition  prevents  less  urgent  behavior  patterns 
from  interfering  with  emergency  responses  critical  for  es- 
caping from,  or  repelling,  mortal  threats  (Wall,  1979; 
Bolles  and  Fanselow,  1980).  In  Aplysia,  strong  shock  or 
pinching  stimuli  inhibit  withdrawal  reflexes  and  associated 
neural  activity  in  an  intensity-dependent  manner,  and  the 
inhibitory  effects  generally  last  for  1  to  15  min  (Marcus 
et  ai.  1988;  Walters,  Erickson,  and  Clatworthy,  unpub. 
obs.).  This  time  course  and  intensity  dependence  roughly 
parallel  those  of  escape  locomotion  (Walters  and  Erickson, 
1986).  Because  massive  withdrawal  of  any  region  of  the 
body  interferes  with  escape  locomotion,  a  major  function 
of  nociceptive  inhibition  in  this  animal  is  probably  to 
prevent  the  disruption  of  escape  behavior  that  would  occur 
if  strong  withdrawal  responses  were  triggered  during  flight. 
As  described  above,  inhibition  of  defensive  responses  ap- 
pears to  involve  neuromodulation  of  sensory  neurons,  in- 
terneurons, and  perhaps  motor  neurons,  with  some  of  the 
inhibition  being  activity-dependent  (Table  I). 


Phase  3 — rcciiperal  it  >n 

After  several  minutes  of  escape  locomotion,  Aplysia 
stop  (in  a  crevice  if  available),  contract  into  a  tight  spher- 
ical shape,  and  remain  motionless.  If  the  injury  is  severe, 
an  animal  may  show  little  sign  of  activity  for  up  to  several 
days.  If  the  animal  is  touched  during  this  time,  it  will 
show  exaggerated  withdrawal  responses  and  a  low  thresh- 
old for  escape  locomotion,  especially  if  contact  is  made 
near  the  wound  (Walters,  1987a,  and  unpub.  obs.).  In- 
activity during  wound  healing  presumably  involves  in- 
hibition of  circuits  controlling  active  behaviors,  such  as 
feeding  and  mating,  which  are  not  immediately  essential 
and  which  would  subject  the  wound  to  further  stress.  In- 
hibitory signals  may  include  neuroendocrine  substances 
and  factors  released  into  the  blood  from  ruptured  cells  at 
the  site  of  trauma  (Krontiris-Litowitz  et  ai.  1989). 

While  little  is  known  about  mechanisms  underlying 
inhibition  of  nonessential  behaviors  during  the  recuper- 
ative phase,  a  great  deal  has  been  learned  about  the  en- 
hancement of  defensive  responses  during  this  phase.  The 
various  mechanisms  of  sensitization  reviewed  earlier  in 
this  article  serve  to  increase  the  animal's  readiness  for 
defensive  action  while  the  wound  heals  (Table  I).  This 
sensitization  is  functionally  equivalent  to  long-term  hy- 
peralgesia  in  mammals.  Hypersensitivity  is  especially  im- 
portant around  the  region  of  injury  because  a  wound  may 
leak  substances  that  can  invite  further  attack  from  pred- 
ators or  parasites,  and  because  a  wounded  region  is  likely 
to  be  weakened  and  vulnerable  to  further  disturbance. 
Persistent  general  sensitization  in  Aplysia  is  mediated,  at 
least  in  part,  by  long-term  heterosynaptic  facilitation  of 
wide-dynamic  range  nociceptors  (Frost  et  ai.  1985;  Wal- 
ters, 1987b).  Wound-specific  sensitization  involves  at  least 
three  basic  mechanisms.  First,  long-term  site-specific  sen- 
sitization is  produced  by  ADEM  of  nociceptors  activated 
during  wounding.  This  selectively  decreases  peripheral 
mechanosensory  threshold  (Billy  and  Walters,  1989a), 
enhances  nociceptor  afterdischarge,  and  produces  synaptic 
facilitation  in  sensory  neurons  innervating  the  wounded 
region  (Walters,  1987b).  Second,  signals  generated  at  a 
site  of  axonal  injury  may  be  carried  by  retrograde  axonal 
transport  to  the  soma  and  synapses,  where  they  induce 
the  same  set  of  hyperexcitability  and  facilitatory  effects 
as  are  triggered  by  ADEM  (Walters,  Alizadeh,  and  Castro, 
unpub.  obs.).  The  generation  of  signals  at  sites  of  axonal 
injury  may  involve  interactions  with  extracellular  factors 
associated  with  immunocytes  aggregating  at  damaged  tis- 
sue (Alizadeh  et  ai,  1990).  In  each  case,  persistent  sen- 
sitization may  involve  growth  of  new  synapses  and 
sprouting  of  new  branches  from  central  and  peripheral 
sensory  arbors  (Bailey  and  Chen,  1988;  Billy  and  Walters, 
1989a).  A  third  basic  mechanism  of  long-term  wound- 
specific  sensitization  has  been  implicated  by  behavioral 


248 


E.  T.  WALTERS 


experiments  (Fig.  2),  but  has  not  yet  been  demonstrated 
within  the  nervous  system — selective  enhancement  of  the 
responsiveness  of  elements  within  motor  control  circuits 
controlling  specific  defensive  responses  appropriate  for 
the  wounded  region  (motor  facilitation;  Erickson  and 
Walters,  1988). 

An  interesting  feature  of  nociceptive  systems  in  Aplysia 
and  in  mammals  is  the  prominence  of  wide-dynamic 
range  neurons.  Results  from  Aplysia  suggest  that  this  fea- 
ture may  be  important  for  the  induction  of  nociceptive 
behavior  by  innocuous  stimuli  during  nociceptive  sensi- 
tization  (an  effect  functionally  equivalent  to  allodynia  in 
humans — pain  evoked  by  innocuous  stimuli).  In  Aplysia 
the  severity  of  an  injury  is  partially  encoded  by  the  total 
output  of  nociceptors  representing  the  injured  region  (i.e., 
the  number  of  cells  activated  X  firing  rate  per  cell  X  trans- 
mitter release  per  spike).  Thus,  a  moderately  intense 
stimulus  that  would  normally  be  innocuous  will  be  in- 
terpreted by  the  CNS  as  noxious  if  transmitter  release  or 
spike  frequency  are  enhanced  in  wide-dynamic  range  no- 
ciceptors after  an  injury.  Presumably,  innocuous  tactile 
stimulation  near  a  serious  wound  is  often  sufficiently 
threatening  to  evoke  nociceptive  behavior  during  recu- 
peration in  both  Aplysia  and  mammals. 

The  ADEM  mechanism  in  LE  and  VC  sensory  neurons 
may  contribute,  not  only  to  site-specific  sensitization 
around  a  wound,  but  also  to  classical  conditioning  of  a 
cutaneous  warning  cue  distant  from  a  wound,  provided 
that  the  warning  cue  is  at  least  moderately  intense  and  is 
delivered  shortly  before  wounding  (Walters  and  Byrne. 
1983;  Hawkins  ct  at..  1983).  However,  this  cue-specific 
sensitization  mechanism  would  only  be  useful  if  the  cue 
were  subsequently  to  contact  the  same  receptive  field. 
Given  the  small  size  of  these  cells'  receptive  fields  (de- 
creasing the  chances  of  repeated  contact),  conditioned 
enhancement  of  their  signals  would  seem  to  provide  an 
undependable  warning  cue  (see  Walters,  1987b).  Cue- 
specific  sensitization  mechanisms  should  be  more  effective 
in  sensory  neurons  that  have  global  receptive  fields  and 
that  can  detect  a  threat  at  a  distance  (before  contact), 
allowing  more  time  for  avoidance  of  a  threatening  situ- 
ation. Chemosensory  neurons  have  these  properties,  and 
chemical  stimuli  may  thus  be  more  effective  cues  for 
aversive  conditioning  than  tactile  cues.  Aversive  condi- 
tioning with  chemosensory  cues  occurs  readily  in  Aplysia 
(Walters  el  ai.  1981;  Colwill  et  ai.  1988),  but  whether 
such  conditioning  is  more  rapid  or  potent  than  condi- 
tioning with  tactile  cues  is  not  yet  known  (e.g.,  Carew  et 
ai.  1981). 

A  Hypothesis  About  the  Evolution 
of  Nociceptive  Plasticity 

Although  nociceptive  neurons  and  nociceptive  re- 
sponses have  been  examined  in  a  variety  of  species  (e.g.. 


Nicholls  and  Baylor.  1968;  Kavaliers,  1988),  investigations 
of  behavioral  and  neuronal  plasticity  following  noxious 
stimulation  have  largely  been  restricted  to  mammalian 
and  molluscan  preparations.  Nociceptive  plasticity  in 
these  two  groups  shows  a  number  of  interesting  similarities 
(reviewed  by  Walters.  1987a,b;  Kavaliers,  1988;  Woolf 
and  Walters,  1991).  In  both  groups,  facilitatory  and  in- 
hibitory alterations  occur  in  the  first  stages  of  nociceptive 
processing — within  wide  dynamic  range  nociceptors  in 
Aplysia.  and  in  primary  nociceptors  and  secondary  wide- 
dynamic  range  spinal  interneurons  in  mammals.  Simi- 
larities include:  intensity-dependent  enhancement  and 
inhibition  of  central  excitability,  enhanced  peripheral 
sensitivity,  enlargement  of  nociceptive  receptive  fields,  and 
activity-dependent  plasticity.  Furthermore,  preliminary 
evidence  suggests  that  aspects  of  the  underlying  subcellular 
mechanisms  (e.g.,  depressed  K+  conductances,  mediation 
by  common  protein  kinases,  and  activation  of  "imme- 
diate-early" genes)  might  also  be  shared  (see  Woolf  and 
Walters,  1991).  In  principle,  the  similarity  of  any  given 
feature  may  be  due  to  either  convergent  evolution  of  in- 
dependent mechanisms  in  response  to  common  environ- 
mental pressures  (analogy),  or  to  conservation  of  primitive 
mechanisms  that  had  evolved  in  ancestors  common  to 
both  mollusks  and  mammals  (homology).  The  ancestors 
of  mollusks  and  mammals  diverged  very  early  in  the  his- 
tory of  the  animal  kingdom,  before  the  protostome  and 
deuterostome  lineages  split  during  the  Precambrian  era. 
Thus,  homologous  features  in  mollusks  and  mammals 
must  be  very  primitive,  having  descended  from  small, 
soft-bodied  animals  that  lived  more  than  600  million  years 
ago,  before  the  hard  shells  or  skeletons  that  would  leave 
a  fossil  record  had  evolved  (Avers,  1989). 

To  what  extent  are  mechanisms  of  nociceptive  plasticity 
homologous  in  mollusks  and  mammals?  Undoubtedly 
many  similarities  are  due  to  analogous  adaptations  de- 
veloped independently  by  these  groups  in  response  to  a 
ubiquitous  pressure — the  dangers  that  follow  sublethal 
injury  in  a  hostile  environment.  On  the  other  hand,  two 
arguments  suggest  that  this  same  pressure  existed  during 
the  evolution  of  primitive  common  ancestors  of  mollusks 
and  mammals,  and  could  have  supported  the  early  evo- 
lution of  adaptations  to  optimize  defensive  behavior  fol- 
lowing noxious  stimulation.  First,  the  presence  of  pred- 
ators during  early  periods  of  animal  evolution  is  suggested 
by  the  occurrence  of  withdrawal  and  escape  responses  in 
virtually  all  existing  animal  groups,  including  protozoans 
(Kavaliers,  1988).  Unfortunately,  almost  nothing  is  known 
about  predators  in  the  Precambrian  world  except  that 
they,  like  their  prey,  would  have  been  small  and  soft-bod- 
ied, and  probably  lacked  specialized  feeding  appendages 
(Vermeij,  1987).  Nevertheless,  Precambrian  predators 
probably  existed  (Hickman  el  ai.  1984)  and  would  not 
have  needed  specialized  appendages.  For  example,  prey 


NOCICEPTIVE  PLASTICITY  IN  APLYSIA 


249 


may  have  been  captured  with  nets  of  mucus,  and  eaten 
with  simple  grasping  and  extracellular  digestion  methods, 
similar  to  those  used  by  some  flatworms  today.  Second, 
injury  could  have  also  been  produced  by  random  assaults 
from  the  environment,  such  as  wave  action.  An  injured 
animal  would  have  been  more  vulnerable  to  further  phys- 
ical disturbance,  and  to  detection  and  attack  by  predators. 

If  early  mechanisms  of  nociceptive  plasticity  did,  in 
fact,  originate  in  primitive  animals  having  very  simple 
nervous  systems,  an  attractive  possibility  is  that  some  of 
these  mechanisms  first  appeared  in  primary  mechanosen- 
sory  neurons.  These  cells  were  among  the  earliest  neurons 
(e.g..  Bullock  and  Horridge,  1965).  Because  they  are  di- 
rectly exposed  to  surface  trauma,  they  provide  a  single 
locus  for  both  recognizing  noxious  stimulation  and  alter- 
ing the  responses  of  an  animal  to  subsequent  mechanical 
stimulation.  Exposure  of  the  peripheral  branches  of 
mechanosensory  neurons  to  surface  trauma  suggests  a 
specific  cellular  hypothesis  for  the  origin  of  some  mech- 
anisms of  nociceptive  plasticity.  Trauma  to  the  body  sur- 
face (or  even  wear  on  soft  body  parts  in  a  turbulent  en- 
vironment) can  damage  peripheral  sensory  branches.  The 
ubiquity  of  cellular  repair  processes  in  modern  cells  sug- 
gests that  such  processes  appeared  very  early  in  the  evo- 
lution of  life  and  were  available  to  repair  damaged  sensory 
branches  in  primitive  animals.  For  an  organism  to  take 
mechanisms  that  had  evolved  to  regenerate  and  maintain 
excitability  in  damaged  neuronal  branches,  and  use  these 
mechanisms  in  undamaged  neurons  where  they  could 
amplify  the  neurons'  normal  signalling  effectiveness, 
seems  a  small  step.  For  example,  mechanisms  of  cellular 
repair,  growth,  and  signal  compensation  triggered  by  in- 
tracellular  signals  of  cellular  injury  might  become  induc- 
ible  by  extracellular  neuromodulators  released  during 
noxious  stimulation,  or  by  interactions  of  such  neuro- 
modulators with  spike  activity  in  the  neuron  (i.e.,  ADEM). 

Recently  we  tested  whether  a  close  relationship  exists 
between  mechanisms  of  general  and  site-specific  sensiti- 
zation  on  the  one  hand  (i.e.,  involving  neuromodulation 
and  ADEM),  and  responses  to  axonal  injury  on  the  other 
(Walters,  Alizadeh,  and  Castro,  unpub.  obs.).  We  studied 
the  effects  of  axonal  injury  by  crushing  nerves  containing 
sensory  neuron  axons  under  conditions  in  which  synaptic 
release  of  neuromodulators  (such  as  5-HT)  and  ADEM 
were  blocked.  Tests  of  the  sensory  neurons  after  nerve 
crush  revealed  profound  hyperexcitability  and  synaptic 
facilitation,  lasting  weeks,  that  were  specific  to  cells  with 
axons  in  the  crushed  nerves.  The  long  latency  of  the  effects 
(1-2  days)  suggested  that  signals  from  axonal  damage  were 
conveyed  back  to  the  soma  and  synapses  by  axonal  trans- 
port. Of  particular  interest  was  the  qualitative  identity  of 
the  set  of  changes  produced  by  axon  crush  with  the  al- 
terations produced  by  neuromodulation  and  ADEM.  One 
interpretation  of  these  results  is  that  a  common  set  of 


mechanisms  underlying  long-term  hyperexcitability  and 
synaptic  facilitation  can  be  triggered  by  two  signalling 
pathways  from  the  periphery  to  the  soma.  The  more 
primitive  pathway  would  be  provided  by  intracellular  sig- 
nals conveyed  slowly  by  axonal  transport  from  damaged 
axons.  ADEM  (conjoint  electrical  activation  and  extrinsic 
neuromodulation)  of  the  soma  during  injury  would  pro- 
vide a  much  more  rapid  signal  of  peripheral  injury  in 
nociceptors  activated  by  injurious  stimuli  (see  Billy  and 
Walters,  1989a).  ADEM  may  have  evolved  later,  when 
the  increased  size  of  animals  and  resulting  distances  be- 
tween nociceptor  somata  and  their  receptive  processes  se- 
lected for  faster  signals,  so  that  regulation  of  protein  syn- 
thesis necessary  for  injury-induced  plasticity  would  not 
be  delayed. 

Although  highly  speculative,  this  hypothesis  and  our 
preliminary  indications  of  links  between  cellular  injury 
responses  and  long-term  sensitization  in  Aplysia  raise  the 
possibility  that  some  of  the  earliest  forms  of  memory  may 
have  evolved  from  cellular  repair  and  signal  compensation 
mechanisms  in  primitive  mechanosensory  neurons.  If.  as 
brains  became  increasingly  complex,  these  cells  served  as 
evolutionary  precursors  of  other  neuronal  types,  then 
primitive  mechanisms  of  nociceptive  plasticity  might  have 
been  important  for  the  evolution  of  other  forms  of  mem- 
ory as  well.  Indeed,  both  axon  damage  (e.g..  Janig,  1988; 
Kelly  el  a/.,  1988)  and  learning  (e.g.,  Disterhoft  el  al. 
1986)  have  been  associated  with  long-term  hyperexcita- 
bility in  mammalian  neurons. 

Evolutionary  arguments  have  obvious  weaknesses,  es- 
pecially when  they  deal  with  eras  that  left  almost  no  fossil 
record.  Nevertheless,  such  arguments  can  lead  to  fresh 
perspectives  and  novel  physiological  and  molecular  pre- 
dictions. The  present  hypothesis  makes  three  testable  pre- 
dictions: ( 1 )  that  common  cellular  mechanisms  (involving 
homologous  molecules)  contribute  to  nociceptive  sensi- 
tization in  a  broad  range  of  species;  (2)  that  critical  mo- 
lecular steps  are  shared  with  mechanisms  involved  in  cel- 
lular repair  and  signal  compensation  following  axonal 
damage;  and  (3)  that  some  of  these  molecular  steps  are 
shared  with  mechanisms  involved  in  traditional  forms  of 
learning  and  memory.  Systematic  comparison  of  learning- 
related  mechanisms  and  injury-related  mechanisms  in  di- 
verse animals  will  put  these  predictions  to  the  test,  and 
may  provide  insight  into  the  evolution  of  some  forms  of 
memory. 

Acknowledgments 

I  am  grateful  to  Dr.  Andrea  Clatworthy  and  Mr.  Chris 
Hickie  for  their  comments  and  for  allowing  me  to  discuss 
some  of  their  unpublished  data.  I  also  thank  Dr.  Bill  Frost 
for  his  comments  and  Mr.  Jim  Pastore  and  Ms.  Linda 
Eshelman  for  preparing  the  illustrations.  Supported  by 


250 


E.  T.  WALTERS 


National  Institute  of  Mental  Health  grant  MH38726  and 
National  Science  Foundation  grant  BNS901 1907. 

Literature  Cited 

Abrams,  T.  \V.,  and  E.  R.  Kandel.  1988.  Is  contiguity  detection  in 
classical  conditioning  a  system  or  a  cellular  property?  Learning  in 
Aplvsia  suggests  a  possible  molecular  site.  Trends  Neurosd.  11:1 28- 
135. 

Alizadeh,  H.,  Clatworthy,  A.  L.,  Castro,  G.  A.,  and  E.  T.  Walters. 
1990.     Induction  of  an  immune  reaction  in  Aplysia  is  accompanied 
by  long-term  enhancement  of  sensory  neuron  excitability.  Soc.  Neu- 
msci.  Abslr  15:  597. 
Avers,  C.  J.,  1989.     Process  and  Pattern  in  Evolution.  Oxford  University 

Press,  New  York.  590  pp. 

Bailey,   C.   H.,   V.   F.   Castellucci,   J.   Koester,   and   E.   R.   Kandel. 
1979.     Cellular  studies  of  peripheral  neurons  in  siphon  skin  of  Aplysia 
ca/ifornica.  J  Neurophysiol.  42:  530-557. 
Bailey,  C.  H.,  and  M.  Chen.  1983.     Morphological  basis  of  long-term 

habituation  and  sensitization  in  Aplysia.  Science  220:  91-93. 
Bailey,  C.  H.,  and  M.  Chen.  1988.     Long-term  memory'  in  Aplysia 
modulates  the  total  number  of  varicosities  of  single  identified  sensory 
neurons.  Proc.  Nat.  Acad.  Sci.  USA  85:  2373-2377 
Barzilai,  A.,  T.  E.  Kennedy,  J.  D.  Sweatt,  and  E.  R.  Kandel.  1989.     5- 
HT  modulates  protein  synthesis  and  the  expression  of  specific  proteins 
during  long-term  facilitation  in  Aplysia  sensory  neurons.  Neuron  2: 
1577-1586. 

Baxter,  D.  A.,  and  J.  H.  Byrne.  1989.  Serotonergic  modulation  of  two 
potassium  currents  in  the  pleural  sensory  neurons  of  Aplysia.  J.  Neu- 
ropliysiol.  62:  665-679. 

Belardetti,  F.,  E.  R.  Kandel,  and  S.  A.  Siegelbaum.  1987.  Neuronal 
inhibition  by  the  peptide  FMRFamide  involves  opening  of  S  K+ 
channels.  Nature  325:  153-156. 

Billy,  A.  J.,  and  E.  T.  Walters.  I989a.  Long-term  expansion  and  sen- 
sitization of  mechanosensory  receptive  fields  in  Aplysia  support  an 
activity-dependent  model  of  whole-cell  sensory  plasticity.  J.  Neurosd. 
9:  1254-1262. 

Billy,  A.  J.,  and  E.  T.  Walters.  1989b.     Modulation  of  mechanosensory 

threshold  in  Aplysia  by  5-HT,  small  cardioactive  peptideB  (SCPB). 

FMRFamide,  acetylcholine,  and  dopamine.  Neurosci  Lett  105:  200- 

204. 

Bolles,  R.  C,  and  M.  S.  Fanselow.  1980.     A  perceptual-defensive-re- 

cuperative  model  of  fear  and  pain.  Behav.  Brain  Sd.  3:  291-323. 
Braha,  O.,  N.  Dale,  B.  Hochner,  M.  Klein,  T.  W.  Abrams,  and  E.  R. 
Kandel.  1990.  Second  messengers  involved  in  the  two  processes  of 
presynaptic  facilitation  that  contribute  to  sensitization  and  dishabit- 
uation  in  Aplysia  sensory  neurons.  Proc.  Nat.  Acad.  Sd.  USA  87: 
2040-2044. 

Bullock,  T.  H.,  and  G.  A.  Horridge.  1965.     Structure  and  Function  in 
the  Nemnis  Systems  of  Invertebrates.  Vol.  1.  W.  H.  Freeman  and 
Co.,  San  Francisco.  798  pp. 
Byrne,  J.  H.,  1980.     Identification  of  neurons  contributing  to  presynaptic 

inhibition  in  Aplysia  ca/ifornica.  Brain  Res  199:  235-239. 
Byrne,  J.  H.,  1983.     Identification  and  initial  characterization  of  a  cluster 
of  command  and  pattern-generating  neurons  underlying  respiratory 
pumping  in  Aplvsia  cali/ornica.  J.  Neurophysiol.  49:  491-508. 
Byrne,  J.  H.,  1987.     Cellular  analysis  of  associative  learning.  Physio/. 

Re\:  67:  329-439. 

Byrne,  J.  H.,  V.  Castellucci,  and  E.  R.  Kandel.  1974.  Receptive  fields 
and  response  properties  of  mechanoreceptor  neurons  innervating  si- 
phon skin  and  mantle  shelf  in  Aplysia.  J.  Neurophysiol.  37:  1041- 
1064. 

Byrne,  J.  H.,  V.  E.  Castellucci,  T.  J.  Carew,  and  E.  R.  Kandel. 
1978.  Stimulus-response  relations  and  stability  of  mechanoreceptor 


and  motor  neurons  mediating  defensive  gill-withdrawal  reflex  in 
Aplvsia.  J.  Neurophysiol.  41:  402-417. 

Carew,  T.  J.,  V.  F.  Castellucci,  and  E.  R.  Kandel.  1971.  An  analysis 
of  dishabituation  and  sensitization  of  the  gill-withdrawal  reflex  in 
Aplvsia.  Intern.  J  Neurosci.  2:  79-98. 

Carew,  T.  J.,  R.  D.  Hawkins,  and  E.  R.  Kandel.  1983.  Differential 
classical  conditioning  of  a  defensive  withdrawal  reflex  in  Aplysia  cal- 
ijornica.  Science  219:  397-400. 

Carew,  T.  J.,  and  E.  R.  Kandel.  1977.  Inking  in  Aplysia  calitormca. 
III.  Two  different  synaptic  conductance  mechanisms  for  triggering 
central  program  for  inking.  /.  Neurophysiol  40:  721-734. 
Carew,  T.  J.,  E.  T.  Walters,  and  E.  R.  Kandel.  1981.  Classical  condi- 
tioning in  a  simple  withdrawal  reflex  in  Aplysia  calilornica.  J.  Neu- 
rosci. 1:  1426-1437. 

Clark,  G.  A.,  and  E.  R.  Kandel.  1984.  Branch-specific  heterosynaptic 
facilitation  in  Aplysia  siphon  sensory  cells.  Proc.  Nat.  Acad.  Sd.  USA 
81:  2577-2581. 

Clatworthy,  A.  L.,  and  E.  T.  Walters.  1988.  Sensitization  and  sensory 
signals  in  Aplysia  II.  Modulation  of  central  bursting  and  spike  con- 
duction. Soc.  Neurosci.  .Ihslr  14:  609. 

Clatworthy,  A.  L.,  and  E.  T.  Walters.  1989.     Activity-dependent  block 
of  central  sensory  conduction  during  inhibition  of  tail  withdrawal 
reflex  in  Aplysia.  Sue  Neurosci-  Abxtr.  15:  1263. 
Clatworthy,  A.  L.,  and  E.  T.  Walters.  1990.     Modulation  of  the  excit- 
ability of  peripheral  receptive  fields  and  axons  in  sensory  neurons  of 
Aplvsia.  Soc.  Neurosci.  Abstr.  16:  20. 
Colwill,  R.  M.,  R.  A.  Absher,  and  M.  L.  Roberts.  1988.     Context-US 

learning  in  Aplysia  ca/itornica  J  Neurosci.  8:  4434-4439. 
Dash,  P.  K.,  B.  Hochner.  and  E.  R.  Kandel.  1990.     Injection  of  the 
cAMP-responsive  element  into  the  nucleus  of  Aplysia  sensory  neurons 
blocks  long-term  facilitation.  Nature  345:  718-721. 
Disterhoft,  J.  F.,  D.  A.  Coulter,  and  D.  L.  Alkon.  1986.     Conditioning- 
specific  membrane  change  of  hippocampal  neurons  in  vitro.  Proc. 
Nat.  Acad.  Sci.  USA  83:  2733-2737. 

Edmonds,  B.,  M.  Klein,  N.  Dale,  and  E.  R.  Kandel.  1990.  Contributions 
of  two  types  of  calcium  channels  to  synaptic  transmission  and  plas- 
ticity. Science  250:  1142-1147. 

Erickson,  M.  T.,  and  E.  T.  Walters.  1988.     Differential  expression  of 
pseudoconditioning  and  sensitization  by  siphon  responses  in  Aplysia: 
novel  response  selection  after  training.  J.  Neurosd.  8:  3000-3010. 
Eskin,  A.,  K.  S.  Garcia,  and  .1.  H.  Byrne.  1989.     Information  storage 
in  the  nervous  system  of  Aplysia:  specific  proteins  affected  by  5-HT 
and  cAMP.  Proc.  Nat.  Acad.  Sci.  USA  86:  2458-2462. 
Fang,  X.,  and  G.  A.  Clark.  1990.     Neural  mechanisms  of  response  spec- 
ificity. I.  Tail  and  mantle  nerve  shock  produce  differential  effects  on 
the  siphon-withdrawal  neuronal  circuit  in  Aplysia.  Soc.  Neurosci. 
Abstr.  16:  596. 

Frost,  W .  N.,  V.  F.  Castellucci,  R.  D.  Hawkins,  and  E.  R.  Kandel. 
1985.  Monosynaptic  connections  made  by  the  sensory  neurons  of 
the  gill-  and  siphon-withdrawal  reflex  in  Aplysia  participate  in  the 
storage  of  long-term  memory  for  sensitization.  Proc.  Nat.  Acad.  Sci. 
USA  82:  8266-8269. 

Frost,  W.  N.,  G.  A.  Clark,  and  E.  R.  Kandel.  1988.  Parallel  processing 
of  short-term  memory'  for  sensitization  in  Aplysia.  J.  Neurobiol.  19: 
297-334. 

Glanzman,  D.  L.,  S.  L.  Mackey,  R.  D.  Hawkins,  A.  M.  Dyke,  P.  E. 
Lloyd,  and  E.  R.  Kandel.  1989.  Depletion  of  5-HT  in  the  nervous 
system  of  Aplysia  reduces  the  behavioral  enhancement  of  gill  with- 
drawal as  well  as  the  heterosynaptic  facilitation  produced  by  tail  shock. 
J.  Neurosd.  9:  4200-4213. 

Hawkins,  R.  D.,  T.  W .  Abrams,  T.  J.  Carew,  and  E.  R.  Kandel.  1983.  A 
cellular  mechanism  of  classical  conditioning  in  Aplysia:  activity-de- 
pendent amplification  of  presynaptic  facilitation.  Science  219:  400- 
404. 


NOC1CEPT1VE  PLASTICITY  IN  APLYSIA 


251 


Hawkins,  R.  D.,  V.  F.  Castellucci,  and  E.  R.  Kandel.  1981.  Interneurons 
involved  in  mediation  and  modulation  of  gill-  withdrawal  reflex  in 
Aplyxia  I.  Identification  and  characterization.  ./  Neurophysiol,  45: 
304-314. 

Hawkins,  R.  D.,  N.  Ealevic,  G.  A.  Clark,  and  E.  R.  Kandel. 
1989.  Classical  conditioning  of  the  Aplyxia  siphon-withdrawal  reflex 
exhibits  response  specificity.  Pwc.  Nat.  Acacl.  Sci.  USA  86:  7620- 
7624. 

Hickie,  C.,  and  E.  T.  Walters.  1990.  Identified  central  motor  neurons 
are  necessary  for  directional  siphon  responses  in  Aplysia.  Soc.  Neu- 
rosci.  Absir.  16:  19. 

I  In  km. in.  C.  P.,  L.  S.  Roberts,  and  F.  M.  Hickman.  1984.  Integrated 
Principles  of  Zoology.  Mosby.  St.  Louis,  Missouri.  1065  pp. 

Janig.  \V.  1988.  Pathophysiology  of  nerve  following  mechanical  injury. 
Pp.  84-108  in  Proceedings  ot  the  I'lli  World  Congress  on  Pain  R. 
Dubner.  G.  F.  Gebhart,  and  M.  R.  Bond.  eds.  Elsevier,  Amsterdam. 

Kandel,  E.  R.,  and  J.  H.  Schwartz.  1982.  Molecular  biology  of  learning: 
modulation  of  transmitter  release.  Science  218:  433-444. 

Kavaliers,  M.  1987.  Evidence  for  opioid  and  non-opioid  forms  of  stress- 
induced  analgesia  in  the  snail.  Cepaea  nemoralis  Brain  Rex.  410: 
111-115. 

Kavaliers,  M.  1988.  Evolutionary  and  comparative  aspects  of  noci- 
ception.  Bruin  Res  Bull  21:923-931. 

Kelly,  M.  E.,  M.  A.  Bisby,  and  K.  Eukowiak.  1988.  Regeneration  re- 
stores some  of  the  altered  electrical  properties  of  axotomized  bullfrog 
B-cells.  J.  .\enrohiol.  19:  357-372. 

Klein,  M.,  J.  Camardo,  and  E.  R.  Kandel.  1982.  Serotonin  modulates 
a  specific  potassium  current  in  the  sensory  neurons  that  show  pre- 
synaptic  facilitation  in  Aplysia.  Proe.  Nat.  Acad.  Set.  VSA  79:  57 1 3- 
5717. 

Klein,  M.,  B.  Hoclmer,  and  E.  R.  Kandel.  1986.  Facilitatory  transmitters 
and  cAMP  can  modulate  accomodation  as  well  as  transmitter  release 
in  Aplvxta  sensory  neurons:  evidence  for  parallel  processing  in  a  single 
cell.  Proe.  Nal.  Acutl.  Sci.  VSA  83:  7994-7998. 

Krontiris-Litowitz,  J.  K.,  B.  F.  Cooper,  and  E.  T.  Walters. 
1989.  Humoral  factors  released  during  trauma  of  Aplyxia.  I.  Body 
wall  contraction,  cardiac  modulation,  and  central  reflex  suppression. 
/  Com/'  Phyxiol.  B  159:  21 1-223. 

Krontiris-Eitowitz,    J.    K.,    M.    T.    Erickson,    and    E.    T.    Walters. 

1987.  Central  suppression  of  defensive  reflexes  in  Aplyxia  by  noxious 
stimulation  and  by  factors  released  from  body  wall.  Soc.  Neuroxci. 
Ahxlr   13:  815. 

Kupfermann,  I.,  V.  Castellucci,  H.  Pinsker,  and  E.  R.  Kandel. 
1970.  Neuronal  correlates  of  habituation  and  dishabituation  of  the 
gill-withdrawal  reflex  in  Aplyxia.  Science  167:  1743-1745. 

Mackey,  S.  L.,  D.  L.  Glanzman,  S.  A.  Small,  A.  M.  Dyke,  E.  R.  Kandel, 
and  R.  D.  Hawkins.  1987.  Tail  shock  produces  inhibition  as  well 
as  sensitization  of  the  siphon-withdrawal  reflex  of  Aplysia:  possible 
behavioral  role  for  presynaptic  inhibition  mediated  by  the  peptide 
Phe-Met-Arg-Phe-NH2.  Proe.  Nat.  Acad.  Sci.  USA  84:  8730-8734. 

Marcus,    E.   A.,   T.   G.   Nolen,   C.   H.   Rankin,   and   T.  J.   Carew. 

1988.  Behavioral  dissociation  of  dishabituation.  sensitization.  and 
inhibition  in  Aplyxia.  Science  241:  210-212. 

Nazif,  F.,  J.  H.  Byrne,  and  L.  J.  Cleary.  1989.  Intracellular  injection 
of  cAMP  produces  a  long-term  (24  hr)  increase  in  the  number  of 


vancosities  in  pleural  sensory  neurons  of  Aplyxia.  Soc.  Neurosci.  Ahstr. 
15:  1283. 

Nicholls,  J.  G.,  and  D.  A.  Baylor.  1968.  Specific  modalities  and  re- 
ceptive fields  of  sensory  neurons  in  the  CNS  of  the  leech.  /  Neuro- 
physiol. 31:  740-756. 

Pinsker,  H.  M.,  W.  A.  Hening,  T.  J.  Carew,  and  E.  R.  Kandel. 
1973.  Long-term  sensitization  of  a  defensive  withdrawal  reflex  in 
Aplysia.  Science  182:  1039-1042. 

Scholz,  K.  P.,  and  J.  H.  Byrne.  1987.  Long-term  sensitization  in  Aplysia: 
biophysical  correlates  in  tail  sensory  neurons.  Science  235:  685-687. 

Sherrington,  S.  C.  1906.  The  Integrative  Action  of  the  Nervous  System 
2nd  Ed.  1947.  University  Press,  Cambridge.  LI.  K.  433  pp. 

Small,  S.  A.,  E.  R.  Kandel,  and  R.  D.  Hawkins.  1989.  Activity-depen- 
dent enhancement  of  presynaptic  inhibition  in  Aplysia  sensory  neu- 
rons. Science  243:  1603-1606. 

Vermeij,  G.  J.  1987.  Evolution  and  Escalation:  An  Ecological  History 
oj  Lite.  Princeton  Llniversity  Press,  New  Jersey. 

Wall,  P.  D.,  1979.     On  the  relation  of  injury  to  pain.  Pain  6:  253-264. 

Walsh,  J.  P.,  and  J.  H.  Byrne.  1989.  Modulation  of  a  steady-state 
Ca2+-activated,  K+  current  in  tail  sensory  neurons  of  Aplysia:  role 
of  5-HT  and  cAMP.  J.  Neurophysiol.  61:  32-44. 

Walters,  E.  T.  1987a.  Site-specific  sensitization  of  defensive  reflexes 
in  Aplrsia:  a  simple  model  of  long-term  hyperalgesia.  J.  Netirose:.  7: 
400-407. 

Walters,  E.  T.  I987b.  Multiple  sensory  neuronal  correlates  of  site- 
specific  sensitization  in  Aplyxia.  ./.  Neurosci.  7:  408-417. 

Walters,  E.  T.  1989.  Transformation  of  siphon  responses  during  con- 
ditioning of .  Iplvsia  suggests  a  model  of  primitive  stimulus-response 
association.  Proe.  Nal.  Acad.  Sci  USA  86:  7616-7619. 

Walters,  E.  T.,  and  J.  H.  Byrne.  1983a.  Associative  conditioning  of 
single  sensory  neurons  suggests  a  cellular  mechanism  for  learning. 
Science  219:  405-408. 

Walters,  E.  T.,  and  J.  H.  Byrne.  I983b.  Slow  depolarization  produced 
by  associative  conditioning  of  Aplysia  sensory  neurons  may  enhance 
Ca2+  entry.  Brain  Res  280:  165-168. 

Walters,  E.  t.,  and  J.  H.  Byrne.  1984.  Post-tetanic  potentiation  in 
Iplvxia  sensory  neurons.  Brain  Res  293:  377-380. 

Walters,  E.  T.,  J.  H.  Byrne,  T.  J.  Carew,  and  E.  R.  Kandel. 
1983a.  Mechanoafferent  neurons  innervating  tail  of  Aplysia.  I.  Re- 
sponse properties  and  synaptic  connections.  J  Neurophysiol.  50: 
1522-1542. 

Walters,  E.  T.,  J.  H.  Byrne,  T.  J.  Carew,  and  E.  R.  Kandel. 
1983b.  Mechanoafferent  neurons  innervating  tail  of  Aplysia.  II. 
Modulation  by  sensitizing  stimulation.  J.  Neurophysiol.  50:  1543- 
1559. 

Walters,  E.  T.,  T.  J.  Carew,  and  E.  R.  Kandel.  1981.  Associative  learning 
in  Aplysia:  evidence  for  conditioned  fear  in  an  invertebrate.  Science 
211:  504-506. 

Walters,  E.  T.,  and  M.  T.  Erickson.  1986.  Directional  control  and  the 
functional  organization  of  defensive  responses  in  Aplysia.  J.  Comp. 
Physiol.  A  159:  339-351. 

Woolf,  C.  J.,  and  E.  T.  Walters.  1991 .  Common  patterns  of  plasticity 
contributing  to  nociceptive  sensitization  in  mammals  and  Aplysia. 
Trends  In  Neurosci.  14:  74-78. 

Wright,  W.  G.,  E.  A.  Marcus,  and  T.  J.  Carew.  1989.  Dissociation  of 
monosynaptic  and  polysynaptic  contributions  to  dishabituation. 
sensitization  and  inhibition  in  Aplysia.  Soc.  Neurosci.  Abstr.  15:  1265. 


Reference:  Biol.  Bull.  180:  252-261.  (April,  1991) 


Neural  Mechanisms  Underlying  Sensitization 
of  a  Defensive  Reflex  in  Aplysia 

L.  J.  CLEARY,  D.  A.  BAXTER,  F.  NAZIF,  AND  J.  H.  BYRNE 

Department  of  Neurobiology  and  Anatomy.  University  of  Texas  Medical  School, 
P.  O.  Box  20708,  Houston,  Texas  77225 


Introduction 

One  of  the  last  frontiers  in  modern  biology  is  under- 
standing the  neural  basis  of  behavior  and  its  modification 
by  processes  such  as  learning.  While  the  ultimate  goal  for 
many  is  to  understand  human  behavior,  other  model  sys- 
tems are  more  commonly  used  in  the  laboratory  with  the 
expectation  that  general  principles  of  neuronal  function 
will  be  conserved  across  phyla.  Mollusks  in  particular  have 
proven  to  be  valuable  model  systems  because  of  their  rel- 
atively simple  and  accessible  nervous  systems.  Aplysia 
califomica  is  a  gastropod  mollusk  with  a  simple  behavioral 
repertoire  and  a  relatively  simple  nervous  system  (see 
Kandel,  1979).  Nevertheless,  a  detailed  understanding  of 
more  complex  phenomena,  such  as  the  modification  of 
behaviors  by  learning  (Kandel  and  Schwartz,  1982)  and 
arousal  (Weiss  et  ai,  1982)  has  also  emerged  from  study 
of  this  animal.  One  class  of  behaviors  that  has  been  studied 
extensively  in  Aplysia  is  that  of  defensive  withdrawal  re- 
flexes. We  have  focused  on  the  tail-siphon  withdrawal  re- 
flex, which  is  elicited  by  mechanical  stimulation  of  the 
tail.  The  features  of  this  reflex  are  similar  to  those  of  the 
siphon-gill  withdrawal  reflex,  which  is  elicited  by  stimu- 
lation of  the  siphon.  Moreover,  both  of  these  reflexes,  or 
their  in  vitro  analogues,  can  be  modulated  in  several  ways, 
including  habituation,  sensitization,  and  classical  condi- 
tioning (Pinsker  et  a!.,  1970;  Carew  et  ai,  1983;  Walters 
and  Byrne,  1983;  Walters  et  at..  1983b).  In  this  paper,  the 
focus  will  be  on  sensitization  of  the  tail-siphon  withdrawal 
reflex. 

Tail-Siphon  Withdrawal  Reflex 

Weak  mechanical  stimulation  of  the  tail  elicits  a  co- 
ordinated contraction  of  the  tail,  siphon,  and  also  the  gill 

Received  18  October  1990:  accepted  24  January  1991. 


(Fig.  1 ;  Walters  et  a/.,  1983a;  Walters  and  Erickson,  1986). 
A  more  intense  stimulus  may  also  elicit  the  release  of  ink 
from  glands  in  the  mantle  cavity.  Weak  electrical  stim- 
ulation through  implanted  electrodes  elicits  contraction 
of  the  tail  and  siphon  (Fig.  2).  The  strength  of  the  reflex 
response  is  estimated  by  measuring  the  duration  of  siphon 
withdrawal.  When  weak  test  stimuli  are  delivered  at  5 
min  intervals,  the  strength  of  the  response  is  fairly  constant 
at  approximately  7  s.  After  delivery  of  a  strong  stimulus 
through  a  hand-held  electrode  over  the  tail  and  lateral 
body  wall,  the  strength  of  the  response  to  subsequent  weak 
stimuli  is  enhanced  significantly.  This  enhancement  is 
called  sensitization.  In  general,  the  time  course  of  sensi- 
tization depends  on  the  training  protocol  and  can  last  for 
a  relatively  short  time,  such  as  15  minutes  to  an  hour 
(Pinsker  a  ai,  1970),  or  for  a  relatively  long  time,  greater 
than  24  hours  (Pinsker  et  ai,  1973;  Frost  et  ai,  1985; 
Scholzand  Byrne,  1987). 

Neural  Circuit  for  Tail-Siphon  Withdrawal 

To  approach  this  phenomenon  at  the  cellular  level,  the 
neurons  that  mediate  the  behavior  must  be  identified  (Fig. 
3).  Sensory  neurons  innervating  the  tail  (TSN)  are  located 
in  the  pleural  ganglion.  The  main  axon  from  these  neurons 
projects  to  the  pedal  ganglion,  but  there  are  numerous 
fine  branches  within  the  pleural  ganglion  as  well.  The  tail 
component  of  the  withdrawal  response  is  mediated  by  a 
monosynaptic  circuit.  Tail  motor  neurons  (TMN)  are  lo- 
cated in  the  pedal  ganglion  (Walters  et  ai,  1983a).  The 
monosynaptic  circuit  is  sufficiently  strong  to  elicit  tail 
withdrawal  (Walters  et  ai.  1983a),  but  other  neurons  also 
contribute.  For  example,  interneurons  (IN)  in  the  pleural 
ganglion  receive  excitatory  input  from  sensory  neurons 
and,  in  turn,  project  to  the  pedal  ganglion  where  they 


252 


NEUROMODULATION  IN  APLYSIA 


253 


A. 


Siphon 


Tail 


Stimulus 


Figure  1.  Dorsal  view  of  Aplysiu  illustrating  the  tail-siphon  with- 
drawal reflex.  (A)  Relaxed.  (B)  Stimulation  of  the  tail  elicits  a  coordinated 
set  of  defensive  responses  including  reflex  withdrawal  of  the  tail,  siphon, 
and  gill. 


excite  motor  neurons,  forming  a  second,  parallel  pathway 
for  tail  withdrawal  (Cleary  and  Byrne,  1985).  The  siphon 
withdrawal  component  of  the  reflex  is  mediated  by  a 
polysynaptic  pathway.  The  population  of  interneurons  in 
the  pleural  ganglion  that  receives  input  from  tail  sensory 
neurons  also  projects  to  the  abdominal  and  cerebral  gan- 
glia. The  siphon  component  of  the  response  is  mediated 
by  motor  neurons  (SMN)  in  the  abdominal  ganglion 
(Perlman,  1979;  Frost  et  a/.,  1988).  Motor  neurons  for 
other  mantle  organs  such  as  the  gill  and  the  ink  gland  are 
located  in  this  ganglion  as  well  (Kupfermann  ft  a!.,  1974; 
Carew  and  Kandel,  1977).  When  stimulated,  the  pleural 
interneurons  excite  siphon  (LSF),  gill  (LDG:),  and  ink 
(L14)  motor  neurons.  In  addition,  they  may  also  trigger 
a  burst  in  the  L25  neurons.  L25  is  a  group  of  pattern- 
generating  neurons  that  appears  to  control  respiratory 
pumping,  a  behavior  that  also  involves  contraction  of  the 
gill  and  siphon  (Byrne,  1983;  Koester,  1989).  L25  neurons 
have  extensive  connections  throughout  the  abdominal 
ganglion.  Therefore,  interneurons  in  the  pleural  ganglion 
appear  to  integrate  sensory  input  from  tail  stimulation 
and  coordinate  the  total  behavioral  response. 

One  characteristic  of  the  connection  between  pleural 
interneurons  and  tail  motor  neurons  is  a  biphasic  exci- 
tation of  the  tail  motor  neurons.  While  a  single  spike  fre- 
quently produces  a  fast  excitatory  postsynaptic  potential 
(EPSP).  a  short  burst  in  the  interneuron  produces  a  long- 
lasting  excitation  that  appears  to  have  two  components 
(Fig.  4).  The  first  component  is  the  fast  EPSP  and  lasts 
for  the  duration  of  the  interneuron  burst,  whereas  the 


B   TESTING  AND  TRAINING  PROTOCOL 


_L 


_L 


Sensitiang  . 
Stimuli 


Siphon 


Figure  2.  Sensitization  of  the  tail-siphon  withdrawal  reflex.  (A)  Sites 
for  delivering  test  and  sensitizing  stimuli.  (B|  Testing  and  training  pro- 
tocol. (C)  Behavioral  results.  A  single  brief  train  (10  s)  of  sensitizing 
stimuli  leads  to  an  enhancement  of  siphon  withdrawal  elicited  by  stimuli 
to  the  tail.  (From  McClendon.  Goldsmith  and  Byrne,  in  prep.) 


second  component  is  a  slow  EPSP  that  can  last  over  one 
minute.  This  slow  EPSP  is  sufficient  in  some  preparations 
to  prolong  the  burst  elicited  by  the  first  component.  In 
addition,  the  slow  EPSP  increases  the  effectiveness  of  syn- 
aptic  input  produced  by  subsequent  stimulation  of  pe- 
ripheral nerves.  Thus,  this  population  of  pleural  inter- 
neurons  plays  a  modulatory  role  by  using  a  conventional 
postsynaptic  mechanism,  temporal  summation  of  excit- 
atory synaptic  inputs. 

Delivery  of  a  sensitizing  stimulus  activates  conventional 
interneurons,  but,  in  addition,  a  separate  modulatory  cir- 
cuit must  be  recruited.  This  circuit  has  not  been  identified 


Figure  3.     Simplified  schematic  diagram  of  the  neural  circuit  con- 
trolling the  tail-siphon  withdrawal  reflex. 


254 


L.  J.  CLEARY  ET  AL. 


IN 


1 


5mV 


]40mV 


10  sec 


Figure  4.  Simultaneous  intracellular  recordings  from  isolated  pleural- 
pedal  ganglia.  A  bnef  (1  s)  depolarization  of  a  pleural  interneuron  (IN) 
elicits  a  long-lasting  depolarization  in  a  tail  motor  neuron  (MN)  in  the 
pedal  ganglion.  The  slow  EPSP  was  of  sufficient  amplitude  to  prolong 
the  high-frequency  burst  of  action  potentials  produced  directly  by  the 
interneuron.  (From  Geary  and  Byrne,  in  prep.) 


for  the  tail-siphon  withdrawal  reflex.  The  pleural  inter- 
neurons,  however,  provide  a  link  to  interneurons  in  the 
abdominal  ganglion  that  modulate  the  neural  circuit  me- 
diating the  gill-siphon  withdrawal  reflex.  The  strength  of 
the  synapse  between  siphon  sensory  and  motor  neurons 
is  enhanced  by  the  activation  of  at  least  three  cells  in  the 
abdominal  ganglion:  L22,  L28,  and  L29  (Hawkins  el  al., 
1981).  One  of  these,  L29,  is  excited  by  pleural  interneurons 
(Cleary  and  Byrne,  1986).  Moreover,  the  pleural  inter- 
neuron to  L29  connections  are  reciprocal.  Thus,  a  positive 
feedback  loop  exists  that  could  prolong  and  enhance  the 
effects  of  a  sensitizing  tail  stimulus. 

Serotonin  as  a  Facilitatory  Transmitter 

Modification  of  the  tail-siphon  withdrawal  reflex  by 
sensitization  is  due  to  changes  in  the  properties  of  neurons 
that  mediate  the  reflex.  The  tail  sensory  neurons  in  par- 
ticular have  been  characterized  as  a  site  of  plasticity  (Wal- 
ters el  al,  1983b;  Scholz  and  Byrne,  1987).  Sensitizing 
stimuli  increase  synaptic  efficacy  and  alter  membrane 
properties  of  the  sensory  neurons,  such  as  resting  mem- 
brane potential,  excitability,  and  action  potential  kinetics 
(Walters  el  al.,  1983b).  These  changes  are  similar  to  those 
occurring  in  siphon  sensory  neurons  as  a  result  of  sensi- 
tization (Castellucci  and  Kandel,  1976;  Kandel  and 
Schwartz,  1982). 

Interneurons  whose  activity  modifies  the  properties  of 
the  tail-siphon  withdrawal  circuit  have  not  been  identified. 
Several  lines  of  evidence  indicate  that  serotonin  is  one  of 
the  modulatory  transmitters  released  by  sensitizing  stim- 
uli. Serotonin  mimics  many  of  the  short-  and  long-term 
effects  of  sensitization.  Serotonin  is  necessary  for  sensi- 
tization to  occur.  In  addition,  serotonergic  circuitry  is 
available  in  the  nervous  system  to  mediate  sensitization. 

Serotonin  mimics  several  of  the  biochemical  and  elec- 
trophysiological  correlates  of  sensitization.  In  a  semi-intact 
preparation,  the  selective  application  of  serotonin  to  the 
pleural-pedal  ganglia  increases  the  intensity  of  the  tail 


withdrawal  reflex,  just  as  the  application  of  a  sensitizing 
stimulus  to  the  tail  does  (Walters  et  al.,  1983b).  At  the 
cellular  level,  serotonin  enhances  the  size  of  the  mono- 
synaptic  EPSP  elicited  in  a  follower  motor  neuron  by  the 
sensory  neuron.  A  slow  depolarization  is  produced  in  the 
sensory  neuron,  and  this  depolarization  is  associated  with 
an  increase  in  membrane  input  resistance  (Ocorr  and 
Byrne,  1985).  Membrane  excitability  is  increased  as  well 
(Baxter  and  Byrne,  1990).  Finally,  serotonin  increases  the 
amplitude  and  duration  of  the  sensory  neuron  action  po- 
tential (Baxter  and  Byrne,  1990). 

The  mechanism  by  which  serotonin  exerts  these  phys- 
iological effects  appears  to  be  due  to  its  ability  to  mimic 
at  least  one  of  the  biochemical  correlates  of  sensitization. 
Using  a  symmetrical  experimental  design,  the  level  of 
cAMP  is  higher  in  the  cell  bodies  of  tail  sensory  neurons 
that  innervate  the  body  wall  exposed  to  sensitizing  stimuli 
than  in  cell  bodies  of  sensory  neurons  innervating  the 
contralateral  unstimulated  side  (Ocorr  et  al.,  1986).  Sim- 
ilarly, application  of  serotonin  to  isolated  clusters  of  sen- 
sory neurons  increases  the  cAMP  content  compared  to 
contralateral  controls  (Ocorr  and  Byrne,  1985;  Pollock  et 
al..  1985;  Sweatt  el  al.,  1989).  The  dose-response  rela- 
tionship indicates  that  the  half-maximal  concentration  of 
serotonin  is  about  1 5  nM. 

To  test  the  hypothesis  that  serotonin  acts  by  activating 
adenylyl  cyclase,  the  effects  of  serotonin  on  two  membrane 
properties,  excitability  and  action  potential  duration,  were 
examined  and  compared  with  the  effects  of  an  analogue 
of  cAMP  (Baxter  and  Byrne,  1990).  Neuronal  excitability 
was  measured  as  the  number  of  action  potentials  elicited 
by  injecting  constant  depolarizing  current  pulses  of  1  s 
duration  and  of  increasing  magnitude.  For  example,  in 
artificial  seawater,  a  test  pulse  of  2  nA  elicited  an  average 
of  three  action  potentials.  Application  of  serotonin  in- 
creased the  average  number  of  spikes  during  the  depolar- 
izing pulse  by  a  factor  of  1 .8.  Application  of  a  membrane- 
permeable,  phosphodiesterase-resistant  analogue  ofcAMP 
(8-4-parachlorophenylthio-cAMP,  8-pcpt-cAMP)  in- 
creased the  excitability  of  the  sensory  neuron  and  doubled 
the  number  of  action  potentials  elicited  by  an  identical 
current  pulse  (Fig.  5A).  Addition  of  serotonin  to  the  bath, 
which  still  contained  the  cAMP  analogue,  failed  to  pro- 
duce a  further  increase  in  excitability.  Thus,  cAMP  has 
potent  effects  on  excitability,  and  these  cAMP-mediated 
effects  are  sufficient  to  fully  account  for  the  effects  of  se- 
rotonin on  neuronal  excitability. 

The  effects  of  8-pcpt-cAMP  on  action  potential  dura- 
tion did  not  parallel  the  effects  of  serotonin,  however. 
Action  potential  duration  was  measured  as  the  time  be- 
tween the  peak  of  the  action  potential  and  its  repolariza- 
tion  to  the  resting  membrane  potential.  Application  of 
the  analogue  of  cAMP  increased  the  duration  of  the  action 


A    Excitability  (2  nA) 
A1      ASW 


NEUROMODULATION  IN  APLYS1A 

B     Action  Potential 


255 


-45  mV  *• 

A2      +CAMP 


0  mV    -  -I  - 


-45  mV  J 

A3        (ASW  +  cAMP)  +  5-HT 


-45  mV 


(ASW  +  cAMP)  +  5-HT 


5  ms 


-45  mV 


l/V 


200  ms 


50  mV 


Figure  5.  Differential  effects  of  an  analogue  ofcAMP  and  5-HT  on  the  duration  of  the  action  potential 
and  excitability  in  tail  sensory  neurons.  Recordings  were  made  from  somata  that  had  been  isolated  surgically 
from  the  pleural  ganglion.  (A)  A  1  s.  2  nA  depolarizing  current  pulse  elicited  three  action  potentials  in 
artifical  seawater  ( A 1 ).  An  identical  current  pulse  elicited  twice  as  many  action  potentials  alter  hath  application 
of  8-pcpt-cAMP  (A2).  The  subsequent  addition  of  serotonin  to  the  bath,  which  still  contained  8-pcpt-cAMP, 
did  not  increase  further  the  number  of  spikes  during  the  pulse  (A3).  (B)  In  the  same  sensory  neuron,  bath 
application  of  8-pcpt-cAMP  (50  pM)  increased  the  duration  of  the  somatic  action  potential  by  a  modest 
amount.  The  subsequent  addition  of  serotonin  (50  pM)  to  the  bath,  which  still  contained  8-pcpt-cAMP. 
dramatically  increased  the  duration  of  the  action  potential.  (From  Baxter  and  Byrne.  1990). 


potential  by  an  average  of  17%  (Fig.  5B).  The  subsequent 
addition  of  serotonin  to  the  bath,  which  still  contained 
the  cAMP  analogue,  further  increased  the  duration  of  the 
action  potential  by  an  additional  230%,  on  average.  Thus, 
cAMP  has  modest  effects  on  spike  broadening  that  are 
not  sufficient  to  occlude  the  effects  of  serotonin. 

The  differential  effects  of  serotonin  and  cAMP  can  be 
accounted  for  by  the  actions  of  these  compounds  on  spe- 
cific membrane  currents.  Use  of  voltage-clamp  and  com- 
puter-subtraction techniques  has  shown  that  cAMP  re- 
duces a  potassium  current,  the  S-current,  that  is  activated 
at  relatively  negative  membrane  potentials,  does  not  in- 
activate and  is  relatively  insensitive  to  block  by  TEA 
(Klein  et  al,  1982;  Pollock  el  ai.  1985;  Shuster  and  Sie- 
gelbaum,  1987;  Baxter  and  Byrne,  1989;  Walsh  and  Byrne, 
1989).  cAMP  activates  a  protein  kinase  that  phosphory- 
lates  the  S-channel  or  a  protein  associated  with  it  (Sie- 
gelbaum  et  al.,  1982;  Shuster  et  al..  1985).  This  phos- 


phorylation  results  in  an  all-or-nothing  closure  of  the 
S-channel  and  thus  a  suppression  of  the  macroscopic  S- 
current.  The  kinetics  of  the  outward  currents  reduced  by 
cAMP  are  qualitatively  similar  at  all  levels  of  depolariza- 
tion. At  low  levels  of  depolarization  (i.e..  —20  mV  or  be- 
low), the  subsequent  addition  of  serotonin  to  the  bath, 
which  still  contained  the  cAMP  analogue,  did  not  produce 
any  further  reduction  in  membrane  current  (Fig.  6A). 
Thus  these  results  and  others  indicate  that  the  suppression 
of  the  S-current  by  5-HT  is  mediated  by  cAMP. 

In  addition,  serotonin  appears  to  modulate  a  second  cur- 
rent in  tail  sensory  neurons  that  is  not  affected  by  cAMP 
and  is  activated  only  at  depolarized  levels  (Baxter  and  Byrne, 
1989,  1990).  The  addition  of  cAMP  changes  the  currents 
produced  by  voltage-clamp  steps  to  +20  mV  by  decreasing 
the  outward  current  at  the  end  of  the  pulse  (Fig.  6B).  Sub- 
sequent addition  of  serotonin  to  the  bath  results  in  a  reduc- 
tion in  outward  current  at  the  beginning  of  the  pulse  and 


256 


L.  J.  CLEARY  KT  AL 

B1     Step  to +20  mV 


A1     Step  to  -20  mV 


a  ASW 

b  +CAMP 

c  (ASW+cAMP)  +  5-HT 


2nA 


a  ASW 

c  (ASW+cAMP)+5-HT 

b  +CAMP 


30  nA 


B2    Difference  Currents 


A2    Difference  Currents 


(I  CAMP) 


(IS.HT) 


1  nA 


-  b    (I  CAMP) 


(IS.HT) 


15nA 
20  ms 

Figure  6.  Differential  effects  of  an  analogue  of  cAMP  and  serotonin  on  membrane  current  in  tail  sensory 
neurons.  In  all  panels,  the  label  for  an  individual  trace  is  aligned  with  the  current  level  at  the  end  of  the 
voltage-clamp  pulse.  (Al )  Current  responses  were  elicited  by  voltage-clamp  pulses  from  —70  to  -20  mV  in 
artificial  seawater  ( inn  c  <;).  after  application  of  8-pcpt-eAMP  (50  n\l)  (mice  b).  and  after  addition  of  serotonin 
(50  iiM)  to  the  bath,  which  still  contained  the  analogue  (trace  c).  Note  that  the  membrane  currents  do  not 
return  to  the  preclamp  level  because  the  cell  was  clamped  from  a  holding  potential  of  -70  to  -20  mV  and 
then  back  to  -50  mV.  (A2)  The  cAMP  difference  current  (IcAMP)  was  isolated  by  subtracting  trace  b  from 
trace  a.  The  cAMP-independent  component  of  the  serotonin  difference  current  (IS.HT)  was  isolated  by  sub- 
tracting /race  c  from  trace  b.  (Bl )  Current  responses  from  the  same  cell  were  elicited  by  voltage-clamp  pulses 
from  -70  to  +20  mV  in  artificial  seawater  (trace  a),  after  bath  application  of  8-pcpt-cAMP  (trace  />)  and 
after  adding  serotonin  to  the  bath,  which  still  contained  the  analogue  (trace  c).  (B2)  The  cAMP  difference 
current  (IcAMP)  was  isolated  by  subtracting  trace  b  from  trace  a.  The  cAMP-independent  component  of  the 
serotonin  difference  current  (I5.HT)  was  isolated  by  subtracting  (race  c  from  trace  />.  The  qualitative  features 
of  the  cAMP  difference  currents  (lc^Mp)  that  were  isolated  from  voltage-clamp  pulses  to  -20  mV  (A2)  and 
+20  mv  (B2)  were  similar  (note  the  change  in  scale).  In  contrast,  the  qualitative  features  of  the  cAMP- 
independent  component  of  the  serotonin  difference  current  (km)  were  vei7  different  at  the  two  potentials. 
At  -20  mV,  the  presence  of  8-pcpt-cAMP  completely  occluded  further  modulation  of  membrane  current 
by  serotonin  (A2).  At  +20  mV,  however,  serotonin  modulated  an  additional  component  of  membrane 
current  (B2).  This  additional  component  probably  represents  modulation  of  the  kinetics  of  the  delayed 
potassium  current.  (From  Baxter  and  Byrne,  1990). 


an  increase  in  outward  current  at  the  end  of  the  pulse.  The 
voltage-dependence  and  sensitivity  to  potassium  channel 
blockers  of  the  cAMP-independent  effects  of  serotonin  sug- 
gest that  the  current  affected  by  serotonin  under  these  con- 


ditions is  the  delayed  potassium  current.  The  effects  of  se- 
rotonin appear  to  be  produced  by  a  mechanism  in  which 
the  kinetics  of  activation  and  inactivation  are  slowed,  rather 
than  one  in  which  the  conductance  of  channels  is  blocked. 


NEUROMODULATION  IN  APLYSIA 


257 


Gl 


Gl 


Figure  7.  Electron  micrograph  through  the  cell  hod>  of  a  pleural 
sensory  neuron  illustrating  a  direct  contact  between  a  serotonergic  process 
and  the  plasma  membrane  ol  the  sensory  neuron.  The  straight  pre-  and 
postsynaptic  membrane  (arrowheads)  and  the  widened  synaptic  cleft 
suggest  that  this  section  is  through  an  active  zone,  a  possible  site  of 
transmitter  release.  Numerous  glial  processes  (G I )  invagmate  the  sensory 
neuron  membrane.  Pleural  ganglia  were  fixed  and  incubated  in  primary 
antisera  to  serotonin  (Incstar.  Inc.)  for  1  week.  Distribution  of  the  antibody- 
was  revealed  by  an  avidin-peroxidase  technique  (Vectastain  ABC,  Vector. 
Inc.).  The  tissue  was  then  osmicated.  embedded  in  plastic,  and  cut  into 
thin  (100  nm)  sections.  The  scale  bar  represents  250  nm.  (From  Zhang. 
Cleary,  Marshak  and  Byrne,  in  prep.) 


Thus,  the  differential  effects  of  serotonin  and  cAMP 
suggest  that  the  cAMP-mediated  modulation  of  the 
membrane  current  is  primarily  responsible  for  the  effects 
of  serotonin  on  neuronal  excitability.  cAMP-independent 
modulation  of  the  delayed  potassium  current  appears  to 
be  primarily  responsible  for  the  effects  of  serotonin  on 
action  potential  duration.  The  mechanism  underlying  this 
component  of  the  serotonin  response  is  not  yet  known. 
Other  second  messenger  pathways  may  be  involved,  how- 
ever. For  example,  protein  kinase  C  appears  to  contribute 
to  presynaptic  facilitation  of  the  siphon  sensory-motor 
synapse  after  it  has  been  depressed  (Hochner  et  al..  1986; 
Braha  el  al..  1990;  Sacktor  and  Schwartz,  1990). 

For  sensitization  to  occur,  intact  serotonergic  neurons 
must  be  present  in  the  nervous  system.  When  serotonin 
is  selectively  depleted  from  the  nervous  system  by  injec- 
tion of  5,7-dihydroxytryptamine  (5,7-DHT).  the  effects 
of  subsequent  sensitizing  stimuli  on  the  siphon-gill  with- 
drawal reflex  are  blocked  (Glanzman  et  ai.  1989).  While 
the  average  amplitude  of  the  EPSP  evoked  in  a  follower 
neuron  by  the  sensory  neuron  is  not  affected  by  5,7-DHT 
treatment,  facilitation  of  PSPs  by  sensitizing  stimuli  is 
drastically  reduced.  These  experiments  do  not  reveal, 
however,  whether  the  endogenous  serotonin  acts  directly 
or  indirectly  on  the  sensory  neurons. 


Evidence  supporting  a  direct  action  of  serotonin  in  the 
tail-siphon  withdrawal  reflex  comes  from  the  observation 
that  serotonergic  fibers  are  in  close  proximity  to  pleural 
sensory  neurons  (Lo  et  al..  1987;  Zhang  et  al..  1988).  Al- 
though there  are  no  serotonergic  neurons  in  the  pleural 
ganglion  itself  (Tritt  et  a/..  1983;  Ono  and  McCaman. 
1984;  Longley  and  Longley,  1986),  there  are  many  sero- 
tonin-containing axons  within  the  neuropil  that  originate 
from  neurons  in  other  ganglia.  Some  of  these  serotonergic 
axons  send  fine  processes  up  to  surround  the  cell  bodies 
of  the  pleural  sensory  neurons.  Subsequent  examination 
using  electron  microscopic  immunocytochemistry  has 
shown  that  these  serotonergic  axons  contain  varicosities 
that  are  in  direct  contact  with  the  plasma  membrane  of 
sensory  neurons  (Fig.  7).  Moreover,  serotonergic  contacts 
may  occur  on  either  the  cell  body  or  the  axon  hillock.  We 
have  not  yet  performed  double-labeling  experiments  to 
examine  the  distribution  of  serotonergic  contacts  along 
the  axons  and  processes  of  sensory  neurons  in  the  neuropil 
of  the  pleural  and  pedal  ganglia. 

Although  neurons  that  give  rise  to  serotonergic  pro- 
cesses in  the  pleural  ganglion  have  not  been  identified, 
there  are  several  candidates.  Most  promising  among  these 
are  the  serotonergic  cells  in  the  cerebral  B  cluster  (Mackey 
el  cil-.  1989;  Cleary  and  Byrne,  unpub.).  These  cells  appear 
to  send  axons  that  project  through  the  cerebral-pleural 
connective  into  the  pleural  ganglion  and  continue  to  the 
abdominal  ganglion  through  the  pleural-abdominal  con- 
nective. Stimulation  of  the  serotonergic  B  cell  produces 
both  spike  broadening  in  siphon  sensory  cells  in  the  ab- 
dominal ganglion  and  facilitation  of  the  PSP  between  si- 
phon sensory  and  follower  motor  neurons. 

We  have  focused  on  the  role  of  serotonin,  but  other 
modulatory  transmitters  may  also  be  involved.  For  ex- 
ample, the  peptide  SCPh  mimics  many  of  the  effects  of 
serotonin,  although  it  binds  to  a  different  receptor 
(Abramsria/..  1984;  Ocorr  et  al..  1986).  Moreover,  some 
identified  neurons  that  produce  facilitation  of  the  synapse 
between  siphon  sensory  and  motor  neurons  do  not  contain 
serotonin  (Kistler  et  al..  1985;  Hawkins  and  Schacher, 
1989).  Future  research  will  be  necessary  to  elaborate  the 
roles  of  other  transmitters  in  sensitization. 

Role  of  the  Cell  Body  in  Modulation 

Activation  of  interneurons  that  provide  serotonergic 
input  to  the  cell  bodies  of  tail  sensory  neurons  may  have 
multiple  modulatory  effects.  For  example,  changes  in  so- 
matic membrane  potential  would  propagate  passively  to 
proximal  axon  branches  within  the  pleural  ganglion.  In 
addition,  elevation  of  cAMP  levels  in  cell  bodies  may  be 
sufficient  to  elevate  cAMP  in  proximal  axonal  branches, 
producing  local  changes  in  membrane  properties  that 


258 


L.  J.  CLEARY  ET  AL. 


could  contribute  to  enhanced  release  of  transmitter  as 
observed  in  siphon  sensory  neurons  (Brunelli  et  a!..  1976; 
Castellucci  ct  ai.  1982).  Because  of  the  limits  imposed 
by  diffusion,  however,  somatic  alterations  may  not  be  suf- 
ficient to  account  for  the  effects  of  sensitizing  stimuli  at 
distal  sites  of  transmitter  release.  Enhancement  by  sero- 
tonin of  synaptic  transmission  at  the  sensory  to  tail  motor 
neuron  synapse  in  the  pedal  ganglion  does  not  require  an 
intact  connection  between  the  axon  of  the  sensory  neuron 
in  the  pedal  ganglion  and  the  cell  body  in  the  pleural 
ganglion  (Hammer  et  ai,  1989).  Similarly,  application  of 
serotonin  to  a  peripheral  synapse  in  the  siphon  selectively 
enhances  transmission  from  that  synapse  (Clark  and 
Kandel,  1984).  Therefore,  facilitation  at  sites  distant  from 
the  cell  body  is  probably  due  to  the  local  action  of  the 
modulatory  transmitter. 

That  the  cell  body  is  functionally  independent  from 
some  regions  of  the  neuron  suggests  that  serotonergic  in- 
put to  the  cell  body  is  specialized  to  activate  mechanisms 
that  are  localized  there.  Mechanisms  that  might  be  reg- 
ulated by  somatic  serotonin  receptors  include  mRNA 
synthesis,  which  is  restricted  to  the  nucleus,  and  protein 
synthesis,  which  is  restricted  to  the  somatic  cytoplasm. 
These  mechanisms  are  of  particular  interest  because  of 
their  role  in  long-term  forms  of  facilitation  and  sensiti- 
zation  (Montarolo  et  ai,  1986;  Castellucci  et  ai.  1989). 

Some  features  of  long-term  sensitization  are  similar  to 
those  of  short-term;  the  major  difference  is  in  the  duration 
of  the  behavioral  change.  To  alter  the  time  course  from 
1  to  24  h  or  more,  however,  a  longer  training  period  is 
required  (Pinsker  et  ai.  1973;  Frost  et  ai.  1985;  Scholz 
and  Byrne,  1987).  Nevertheless,  the  cellular  correlates  of 
long-term  sensitization  are  nearly  identical  to  those  of 
short-term.  The  amplitude  of  EPSPs  at  synapses  between 
siphon  sensory  and  motor  neurons  is  enhanced  (Frost  et 
ai.  1985).  and  membrane  properties  of  tail  sensory  neu- 
rons are  altered  (Scholz  and  Byrne,  1987).  The  similarities 
between  short-  and  long-term  modifications  of  sensory 
neurons  suggest  that  correlates  of  long-term  sensitization 
are  due  to  the  action  of  a  common  mediator.  Several  lines 
of  evidence  suggest  that  cAMP  can  by  itself  produce  long- 
term  changes  in  the  properties  of  sensory  neurons.  Tran- 
sient elevation  of  the  intracellular  level  of  cAMP  leads  to 
altered  membrane  currents  (Scholz  and  Byrne,  1988)  and 
enhanced  synaptic  transmission  (Schacher  et  ai.  1988) 
that  each  persist  for  at  least  24  h. 

An  additional  cellular  correlate  of  long-term  sensiti- 
zation in  siphon  sensory  neurons  is  a  change  in  the  mor- 
phology of  sensory  neuron  axons  and  synapses  (Bailey 
and  Chen,  1983;  Bailey  and  Chen,  1988).  To  examine  its 
role  in  producing  these  changes,  cAMP  was  injected  ion- 
tophoretically,  over  a  period  of  1 5  min,  into  the  cell  bodies 
of  tail  sensory  neurons.  These  neurons  were  subsequently 


cAMP 


5'  AMP 


50pm 


Figure  8.  Camera  lucida  drawings  of  two  HRP-filled  sensory  neurons 
from  the  same  animal.  The  cell  in  the  top  panel  was  injected  with  cAMP 
approximately  24  h  before  fixation;  the  cell  in  the  lower  panel  was  injected 
with  5'-AMP.  In  each  cell,  a  single  large  axon  extends  from  the  cell  body 
to  the  pleural-pedal  connective.  Because  we  analyzed  only  their  number, 
varicosities  (arrows)  were  drawn  slightly  larger  than  scale  to  enhance 
visibility.  Varicosities  counted  in  the  cAMP-filled  cell  were  78  and  in 
the  5'-AMP-nlled  cell  46.  Branch  points  (arrowheads)  counted  in  the 
cAMP-filled  cell  were  32  and  in  the  5'-AMP-filled  cell  24.  Not  all  the 
branch  crossings  in  these  drawings  are  branch  points  since  these  are 
three-dimensional  objects  drawn  in  two  dimensions.  Some  branches  ap- 
pear disconnected  as  a  consequence  of  the  tissue  processing  procedure. 
(From  Nazif  rtu/..  1991.) 


labeled  with  HRP  and  fixed  24  h  after  cAMP  injection 
(Fig.  8).  Close  analysis  of  the  structure  of  these  neurons 
revealed  that  both  the  number  of  varicosities  was  doubled 
and  the  number  of  branch  points  was  increased  by  50% 
compared  to  neurons  that  had  been  injected  with  5'-AMP, 
the  inactive  metabolite  of  cAMP.  The  significance  of  these 
morphological  changes  lies  in  the  hypothesis  that  sensory 
neurons  make  more  synapses  with  follower  neurons  in 
the  pleural  ganglion  as  a  result  of  sensitization.  This  im- 
plies that  connections  between  sensory  neurons  and  their 
followers  are  strengthened.  Additional  followers  might  also 
be  recruited  by  the  formation  of  new  connections. 
Changes  in  both  of  these  morphological  features  could 
contribute  to  the  enhanced  strength  of  the  reflex.  In  future 
experiments,  it  will  be  interesting  to  test  the  role  of  cAMP 
in  producing  other  morphological  changes  such  as  in- 
creases in  the  number,  size,  and  vesicle  complement  of 
synaptic  active  zones  (Bailey  and  Chen,  1983). 


NEUROMODULATION  IN  APLYSIA 


259 


r 


VARICOSITIES 
BRANCH  POINTS 


M 


Figure  9.  Simplified  diagram  illustrating  the  activation  of  parallel 
cAMP-dependent  pathways  by  neurites  that  release  serotonin  (5-HT) 
onto  the  cell  bodies  of  sensory  neurons.  Transient  elevation  of  cAMP 
in  the  soma  produces  rapid  but  short-lasting  effects  on  membrane  con- 
ductance (\i>lul  lines)  via  activation  of  protein  kinase  (PK).  In  addition, 
it  produces  persistent  effects  on  membrane  conductance  and  neuronal 
structure  (dashed lines).  The  mechanism  underlying  these  long-term  ef- 
fects is  not  known,  but  regulation  of  gene  expression  and  protein  synthesis 
appears  to  be  involved. 


Because  cAMP  in  sensory  neurons  is  broken  down  rap- 
idly (Bernier  et  a/.,  1982;  Schwartz  ct  ai.  1983),  long- 
term  changes  must  depend  on  a  more  persistent  cellular 
process.  A  likely  model,  then,  is  one  in  which  the  transient 
cAMP  signal  activates  at  least  two  pathways  (Fig.  9). 
cAMP  produces  a  rapid  but  short-lasting  modulation  of 
membrane  currents,  decreasing  membrane  conductance. 
In  addition,  a  slower  but  more  enduring  mechanism  is 
also  activated,  producing  long-term  changes  in  membrane 
conductance  and  cell  structure.  The  mechanisms  under- 
lying these  long-term  alterations  are  not  known,  but  reg- 
ulatory pathways  that  alter  protein  synthesis  or  gene 
expression  are  presumably  involved.  Indeed,  long-term 
enhancement  of  synaptic  transmission  and  membrane 
excitability  are  affected  by  inhibitors  of  protein  and  RNA 
synthesis  (Montarolo  et  ai,  1986;  Dale  et  ai.  1987; 
Schacher  et  ai,  1988).  In  addition,  serotonin  appears  to 
activate  regulatory  proteins  that  bind  to  DNA  (Dash  et 
ai,  1990).  As  a  consequence,  messenger  RNA  is  synthe- 
sized (Zwartjes  et  ai,  1990),  and  the  incorporation  of 
amino  acids  into  proteins  of  sensory  neurons  is  altered 
(Barzilai  et  ai,  1989;  Eskin  et  ai,  1989).  Other  mecha- 
nisms, such  as  post-translational  modification  of  proteins 
(Greenberg  et  ai.  1987),  may  be  used  as  a  result  of  sen- 
sitization,  providing  an  intermediate  time  course. 

Acknowledgments 

Supported  by  Texas  Higher  Education  Co-ordination 
Board  Grant  1945  (L.J.C.),  National  Research  Service  Award 
F31  MH09956  (F.A.N.),  NIMH  Award  KO2  MH00649 
(J.H.B.),  and  NIH  Grant  RO1  NS19895  (J.H.B.). 


Literature  Cited 

Abrams,  T.  W.,  V.  F.  Caslellucci,  J.  S.  Camardo,  E.  R.  Kandel,  and 
P.  E.  Lloyd.  1984.  Two  endogenous  neuropeptides  modulate  the 
gill  and  siphon  withdrawal  reflex  in  Aplysia  by  presynaptic  facilitation 
involving  cAMP-dependent  closure  of  a  serotonin-sensitive  potassium 
channel.  Pruc  Null  Acad.  Sci.  USA  81:  7956-7960. 

Bailey,  C.  H.,  and  M.  Chen.  1983.  Morphological  basis  of  long-term 
habituation  and  sensitization  in  Aplyxia.  Science  220:  91-93. 

Bailey,  C.  H.,  and  M.  Chen.  1988.  Long-term  memory  in  Aplysia 
modulates  the  total  number  of  varicosities  of  single  identified  sensory 
neurons.  Pruc.  Nail.  Acacl.  Sci.  USA  85:  2373-2377. 

Barzilai,  A.,  T.  E.  Kennedy,  J.  D.  Sweatt,  and  F.  R.  Kandel.  1989.  5- 
HT  modulates  protein  synthesis  and  the  expression  of  specific  proteins 
during  long-term  facilitation  in  Aplysia  sensory  neurons.  Neunm  2: 
1577-1586. 

Baxter,  D.  A.,  and  J.  H.  Byrne.  1989.  Serotonergic  modulation  of  two 
potassium  currents  in  the  pleural  sensory  neurons  of  Aplysia.  J.  Neii- 
rophysiol.  62:  665-679. 

Baxter,  D.  A.,  and  J.  H.  Byrne.  1990.  Differential  effects  of  cAMP  and 
serotonin  on  membrane  current,  action  potential  duration,  and  ex- 
citability in  somata  of  pleural  sensory  neurons  of  Aplysia.  J.  Neu- 
mphyswl.  64:  978-990. 

Bernier,  L.,  V.  F.  Castellucci,  E.  R.  Kandel,  and  J.  H.  Schwartz. 
1982.  Facilitatory  transmitter  causes  a  selective  and  prolonged  in- 
crease in  adenosine  3':5'-monophosphate  in  sensory  neurons  me- 
diating the  gill  and  siphon  withdrawal  reflex  in  Aplysia.  J.  Neurosci. 
2:  1682-1691. 

Braha,  O.,  N.  Dale,  B.  Hochner,  M.  Klein,  T.  W.  Abrams,  and  E.  R. 
Kandel.  1990.  Second  messengers  involved  in  two  processes  of  pre- 
synaptic facilitation  that  contribute  to  sensitization  and  dishabituation 
in  Aplvsia  sensory  neurons.  Pruc.  Nat.  Acad.  Sci.  USA  87:  2040- 
2044. 

Brunelli,  M.,  V.  F.  Castellucci,  and  E.  R.  Kandel.  1976.  Synaptic  fa- 
cilitation and  behavioral  sensitization  in  Aplysia:  possible  role  for 
serotonin  and  cAMP.  Science  194:  1 178-1 181. 

Byrne,  J.  H.  1983.  Identification  and  initial  characterization  of  a  cluster 
of  command  and  pattern-generating  neurons  underlying  respiratory' 
pumping  in  Aplvsia  califomica,  J  Neurophysiol.  49:  491-508. 

Carew,  T.  J.,  R.  D.  Hawkins,  and  E.  R.  Kandel.  1983.  Differential 
classical  conditioning  of  a  defensive  withdrawal  reflex  in  Aplysia  cal- 
ifomica. Science  219:  397-400. 

Carew,  T.  J.,  and  E.  R.  Kandel.  1977.  Inking  in  Aplysia  califomica: 
neural  circuit  of  an  all-or-none  behavioral  response.  /  Neurophysiol. 
40:  692-707. 

Castellucci,  V.  F.,  H.  Blumenfeld,  P.  Goelet,  and  E.  R.  Kandel. 
1989.  Inhibitor  of  protein  synthesis  blocks  long-term  behavioral 
sensitization  in  isolated  gill-withdrawal  reflex  of  Aplysia.  J.  Neurobiol. 
20:  1-9. 

Castellucci,  V.  F.,  and  E.  R.  Kandel.  1976.  Presynaptic  facilitation  as 
a  mechanism  for  behavioral  sensitization  in  Aplysia.  Science  194: 
1176-1178. 

Castellucci,  V.  F.,  A.  Nairn,  P.  Greengard,  J.  H.  Schwartz,  and  E.  R. 
Kandel.  1982.  Inhibitor  of  adenosine  3':5'-monophosphate-depen- 
dent  protein  kinase  blocks  presynaptic  facilitation  in  Aplysia.  J.  Neu- 
rosci. 2:  1673-1681. 

Clark,  G.  A.,  and  E.  R.  Kandel.  1984.  Branch-specific  heterosynaptic 
facilitation  in  Aplysia  siphon  sensory  cells.  Proc.  Nat.  Acad.  Sci.  USA 
81:2577-2581. 

Cleary,  L.  J.,  and  J.  H.  Byrne.  1985.  Interneurons  contributing  to  the 
mediation  and  modulation  of  the  tail  withdrawal  reflex  in  Aplysia. 
Soc.  Neurosci.  Abstr.  11:  692. 


260 


L.  J.  CLEARV  KT  AL. 


deary,  I,.  J.,  and  J.  II.  Byrne.  1986.  Associative  learning  of  the  gill 
and  siphon  withdrawal  reflex  in  .  lpl\'\ia:  interneurons  mediating  the 
unconditioned  reflex.  Soc.  Neurosci.  Ab.t.  12:  397. 

Dale,  N.,  E.  R.  Kandel,  and  S.  Schachcr.  1987.  Serotonin  produces 
long-term  changes  in  the  excitability  ofAplysia  sensory  neurons  in 
culture  that  depend  on  new  protein  synthesis.  J.  Neurosci.  1:  2232- 
2238. 

Dash,  P.  K.,  B.  lloechner,  and  E.  R.  kandel.  1990.  Injection  of  the 
cyclic  AMP  responsive  element  into  the  nucleus  ofAplysia  sensory 
neurons  blocks  long-term  facilitation.  Nature  345:  718-721. 

I  skin.  A.,  K.  S.  Garcia,  and  J.  H.  Byrne.  1989.  Information  storage 
in  the  nervous  system  ofAplysia:  specific  proteins  affected  by  serotonin 
and  cAMP.  Proc.  Nal.  Aaiii.  Sci.  USA  86:  2458-2462. 

Frost,  W.  N.,  V.  F.  Castellucci,  R.  D.  Hawkins,  and  E.  R.  Kandel. 
1985.  Monosynaptic  connections  made  by  the  sensory  neurons  of 
the  gill-  and  siphon-withdrawal  reflex  in  Aplysia  participate  in  the 
storage  of  long-term  memory  for  sensitization.  Proc.  Nat.  Acad.  Sci. 
USA  82:  8266-8269. 

Frost,  W.  N.,  G.  A.  Clark,  and  E.  R.  Kandel.  1988.  Parallel  processing 
of  short-term  memory  for  sensitization  in  Aplysia.  J.  Neurobiol  19: 
297-334. 

Glanzman,  D.  I...  S.  L.  Mackey,  R.  D.  Hawkins,  A.  M.  Dyke,  P.  E. 
Lloyd,  and  E.  R.  Kandel.  1989.  Depletion  of  serotonin  in  the  nervous 
system  ofAplysia  reduces  the  behavioral  enhancement  of  gill  with- 
drawal as  well  as  the  heterosynaptic  facilitation  produced  by  tail  shock. 
J.  Neurosci.  9:  4200-42 1 3. 

Greenberg,  S.  M.,  V.  F.  Castellucci,  H.  Bayley,  and  J.  H.  Schwartz. 
1987.  A  molecular  mechanism  for  long-term  sensitization  in  Aply- 
sia. Nature  329:  62-65. 

Hammer,  M.,  L.  J.  Cleary,  and  J.  II.  Byrne.  1989.  Serotonin  acts  in 
the  synaptic  region  of  sensory  neurons  in  Aplysia  to  enhance  trans- 
mitter release.  Neurosci.  Lett.  104:  235-240. 

Hawkins,  R.  D.,  V.  F.  Castellucci,  and  E.  R.  Kandel.  1981.  Interneurons 
involved  in  mediation  and  modulation  of  gill-withdrawal  reflex  in 
Aplysia  II.  Identified  neurons  produce  heterosynaptic  facilitation 
contributing  to  behavioral  sensitization.  /  Neurophysiol.  45:  315- 
326. 

Hawkins,  R.  D.,  and  S.  Schacher.  1989.  Identified  facilitator  neurons 
L29  and  L28  are  excited  by  cutaneous  stimuli  used  in  dishabituation, 
sensitization.  and  classical  conditioning  of  Aplysia.  ./.  Neurosci.  9: 
4236-4245 

Hochner,  B.,  M.  Klein,  S.  Schacher,  and  E.  R.  Kandel.  1986.  Action- 
potential  duration  and  the  modulation  of  transmitter  release  from 
the  sensory  neurons  of  Aplysia  in  presynaptic  facilitation  and  be- 
havioral sensitization.  Proc.  Nat.  Acad.  Sci.  i'SA  83:  8410-8414. 

Kandel,  E.  R.  1979.  Behavioral  biology  of  Aplysia.  W.  H.  Freeman, 
San  Francisco. 

Kandel,  E.  R.,  and  J.  H.  Schwartz.  1982.  Molecular  biology  of  learning: 
modulation  of  transmitter  release.  Science  218:  433-443. 

Kistler,  H.  B.,  Jr.,  R.  D.  Hawkins,  J.  Koester,  H.  W.  Steinbusch,  E.  R. 
Kandel  and  J.  H.  Schwartz.  1985.  Distribution  of  serotonin-im- 
munoreactive  cell  bodies  and  processes  in  the  abdominal  ganglion 
of  mature  Aplysia.  ./.  Neurosci.  5:  72-80. 

Klein.  M.,  J.  S.  Camardo,  and  E.  R.  Kandel.  1982.  Serotonin  modulates 
a  new  potassium  current  in  the  sensory  neurons  that  show  presynaptic 
facilitation  in  Aplysia.  Proc.  Nat.  Acad.  Sci.  USA  79:  5713-5717. 

Koester,  J.  1989.  Chemically  and  electrically  coupled  interneurons 
mediate  respiratory  pumping  in  Aplysia.  J  Neurophvsiol.  62:  1113- 
1126. 

Kupfermann,  I.,  T.  J.  Carew,  and  E.  R.  Kandel.  1974.  Local,  reflex  and 
central  commands  controlling  gill  and  siphon  movements  in  Aplysia. 
J  Neurophysiol.  37:  996-1019. 


Lo,  L.  T.,  J.  H.  Byrne,  and  L.  J.  Cleary.  1987.  Distribution  of  three 
modulatory  transmitters  within  the  pleural  ganglion  ofAplysia.  Soc. 
Neurosci.  Abstr.  13:  1073. 

I.ongley,  R.  D.,  and  A.  J.  Longley.  1986.  Serotonin  immunoreactivity 
of  neurons  in  the  gastropod  Aplysia  californica.  J.  Neurobiol.  17: 
339-358. 

Mackey,  S.  L.,  E.  R.  Kandel,  and  R.  D.  Hawkins.  1989.  Identified 
serotonergic  neurons  LCB1  and  RCB1  in  the  cerebral  ganglia  of 
Aplysia  produce  presynaptic  facilitation  of  siphon  sensory  neurons. 
J.  Neurosci  9:  4227-4235. 

Montarolo,  P.  G.,  P.  Goelet,  V.  F.  Castellucci,  J.  Morgan,  E.  R.  Kandel, 
and  S.  Schacher.  1986.  A  critical  period  for  macromolecular  syn- 
thesis in  long-term  heterosynaptic  facilitation  in  Aplysia.  Science  234: 
1249-1254. 

Nazif,  F.  A.,  J.  H.  Byrne,  and  L.  J.  Cleary.  1991.  cAMP  induces  long- 
term  morphological  changes  in  sensory  neurons  of  Aplysia.  Brain 
Res.  539:  324-327. 

Ocorr,  K.  A.,  and  J.  II.  Byrne.  1985.  Membrane  responses  and  changes 
in  cAMP  levels  in  Aplysia  sensory  neurons  produced  by  serotonin, 
tryptamine,  FMRFamide  and  small  cardioactive  peptide  B  (SCPB). 
Neurosci.  Lett  55:  1  13-1  IS. 

Ocorr,  K.  A.,  and  J.  II.  Byrne.  1986.  Evidence  for  separate  receptors 
that  mediate  parallel  effects  of  serotonin  and  small  cardioactive  pep- 
tide  B  (SCPB)  on  adenylate  cyclase  in  Aplysia  californica.  Neurosci. 
Lett.  70:  283-288. 

Ocorr,  K.  A.,  M.  Tabata,  and  J.  II.  Byrne.  1986.  Stimuli  that  produce 
sensitization  lead  to  elevation  of  cyclic  AMP  levels  in  tail  sensory 
neurons  of  Aplysia.  Brain.  Res.  371:  190-192. 

Ono,  J.  K.,  and  R.  E.  McCaman.  1984.  Immunocytochemical  local- 
ization and  direct  assays  of  serotonin-containing  neurons  in  Aplysia. 
Newoscicncc  11:  549-560. 

Perlman,  A.  J.  1979.  Central  and  peripheral  control  of  siphon-with- 
drawal reflex  in  Aplysia  californica.  J.  Neurophysiol.  42:  510-529. 

Pinsker,  H.,  I.  Kupfermann,  V.  F.  Castellucci,  and  E.  R.  Kandel. 
1970.  Habituation  and  dishabituation  of  the  gill-withdrawal  reflex 
in  Ap/ysia.  Science  167:  1740-1742. 

Pinsker,  H.  M.,  W.  A.  Hening,  T.  J.  Carew,  and  E.  R.  Kandel. 
1973.  Long-term  sensitization  of  a  defensive  withdrawal  reflex  in 
Aplysia.  Science  182:  1039-1042. 

Pollock,  J.  D.,  L.  Bernier,  and  J.  S.  Camardo.  1985.  Serotonin  and 
cyclic  adenosine  3':5'-monophosphate  modulate  the  potassium  current 
in  tail  sensory  neurons  in  the  pleural  ganglion  ofAplysia.  J.  Neurosci. 
5:  1862-1871. 

Sacktor,  T.,  and  J.  H.  Schwartz.  1990.  Sensitizing  stimuli  cause  trans- 
location  of  protein  kinase  C  in  Aplysia  sensory  neurons.  Proc.  Nat. 
Acad.  Sci.  USA  87:  2036-2039. 

Schacher,  S.,  V.  F.  Castellucci,  and  E.  R.  Kandel.  1988.  cAMP  evokes 
long-term  facilitation  in  Aplysia  sensory  neurons  that  requires  new 
protein  synthesis.  Science  240:  1667-1669. 

Scholz,  K.  P.,  and  J.  H.  Byrne.  1987.  Long-term  sensitization  in  Aplysia: 
biophysical  correlates  in  tail  sensory  neurons.  Science  235:  685-687. 

Scholz,  K.  P.,  and  J.  H.  Byrne.  1988.  Intracellular  injection  of  cAMP 
induces  a  long-term  reduction  of  neuronal  K'  currents.  Science  240: 
1664-1666. 

Schwartz,  J.  H.,  L.  Bernier,  V.  F.  Castellucci,  M.  Palazzolo,  T.  Saitoh, 
A.  Stapleton,  and  E.  R.  Kandel.  1983.  What  molecular  steps  de- 
termine the  time  course  of  the  memory  for  short-term  sensitization 
in  Aplysia?  Cold.  Spring.  Harbor.  Symp.  Quant.  Biol.  48:  8 1 1  -8 1 9. 

Shuster,  M.  J.,  J.  S.  Camardo,  S.  A.  Siegelbaum,  and  E.  R.  Kandel. 
1985.  Cyclic  AMP-dependent  protein  kinase  closes  the  serotonin- 
sensitive  K+  channels  of  Aplysia  sensory  neurons  in  cell-free  mem- 
brane patches.  Naiure  313:  392-395. 


NEUROMODULATION   IN  APLYSIA 


261 


Shuster,  M.  J..  and  8.  A.  Siegelbaum.  1987.  Pharmacological  char- 
acterization of  the  serotonin-sensitive  potassium  channel  of  .-l/>/y.v/d 
sensory  neurons.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  90:  587-608. 

Siegelbaum,  S.  A.,  J.  S.  Camardo,  and  E.  R.  Kandcl.  1982.  Serotonin 
and  cAMP  close  single  K+  channels  in  Aply.siu  sensory  neurons.  Na- 
ture 299:413-417. 

Sweatt,  J.  D.,  A.  Volterra,  B.  Edmonds,  K.  A.  Karl,  S.  A.  Siegelbaum, 
and  K.  R.  kandel.  1989.  FMRFamide  reverses  protein  phosphor- 
ylation  produced  by  5-HT  and  cAMP  in  Aplysia  sensory  neurons. 
Nalurc  342:  275-278. 

Tritt,  S.  H.,  I.  P.  Lowe,  and  J.  H.  Byrne.  1983.  A  modification  of  the 
glyoxylic  acid  induced  histofluorescence  technique  for  demonstration 
of  catecholamines  and  serotonin  in  tissues  of  Aplysia  califomica. 
Brain  Re".  259:  159-162. 

Walsh,  J.  P.  and  J.  H.  Byrne.  1989.  Modulation  of  a  steady-state  Ca2+- 
activated.  K+  current  in  tail  sensory  neurons  of  Aplysitr  role  of  se- 
rotonin and  cAMP.  /  Neurophysiol.  61:  32-44. 

Walters,  E.  T.,  and  J.  H.  Byrne.  1983.  Associative  conditioning  of 
single  sensory  neurons  suggests  a  cellular  mechanism  for  learning. 
Science  219:  405-408. 

Walters,  E.  T.,  J.  H.  Byrne,  T.  J.  Carew,  and  E.  R.  Kandel. 
I983a.  Mechanoafferent  neurons  innervating  tail  of.  -Iplysia  I.  Re- 


sponse properties  and  synaptic  connections.  J  Neurophysiol   50: 
1522-1542. 
Walters,    E.    T.,    J.    II.    Byrne,    T.    J.    Carew,    and    E.    R.    Kandel. 

1983b.     Mechanoafferent  neurons  innervating  tail  of  Afily.ua.  II.  Mod- 
ulation by  sensitizing  stimulation.  ./  Nciirophysml.  50:  1543-1559. 

Walters,  E.  T.,  and  M.  I .  Eriekson.  1986.  Directional  control  and  the 
functional  organization  of  defensive  responses  in  Aplysia.  J  Comp 
Physiol.  A  159:339-351. 

Weiss,  K.  R.,  U.  T.  Koch,  J.  Koester,  S.  C.  Rosen,  and  I.  Kupfermann. 
1982.  The  role  of  arousal  in  modulating  feeding  behavior  ot'Aplysin: 
neural  and  behavioral  studies.  Pp.  25-57  in  The  Neural  BUMS  <>/ 
{•'ceiling  and  Reward.  B.  G.  Hoebel  and  D.  Novin,  ed.  Haer  Institute, 
New  Brunswick,  ME. 

Zhang,  Z.  S.,  L.  J.  Cleary,  1).  M.  Marshak,  and  J.  H.  Byrne. 
1988.  Serotonergic  varicosities  make  apparent  synaptic  contacts 
with  pleural  sensory  neurons  of  Aplysia.  Soc.  Neurosci.  Ahxir  14: 
841. 

/wartjes,  R.  E.,  M.  I .  Crow,  J.  II.  Byrne,  and  A.  Eskin.  1990.  Serotonin 
regulates  the  expression  of  mRNA  in  the  pleural-pedal  ganglia  of 
Aplyxia.  as  determined  by  in  viirn  translation.  Soc.  Neurosci.  Ahslr. 
16:  596. 


Reference:  Biol.  Bull.  180:  262-268.  (April, 


Studies  of  Behavioral  State  in  Aplysia 


IRVING  KUPFERMANN,  THOMAS  TEYKE,  STEVEN  C.  ROSEN, 
AND  KLAUDIUSZ  R.  WEISS* 

Center  for  Neurobialogy  and  Behavior,  Columbia  University  College  of  Physicians  and  Surgeons: 

New  York  State  Psychiatric  Institute:  and  *  Department  of  Physiology  and  Biophysics. 

Aft.  Sinai  School  of  Medicine,  New  York,  New  York 


Abstract.  This  paper  reviews  a  series  of  studies  on  the 
neural  organization  and  the  cellular  mechanisms  under- 
lying behavioral  states;  in  these  studies,  feeding  behavior 
in  Aplysia  was  used  as  a  model  system.  Feeding  in  Aplysia 
has  similarities  to  motivated  behaviors  in  other  animals 
and  is  modulated  by  a  number  of  interesting  state  vari- 
ables, including  arousal.  Food-induced  arousal  manifests 
itself  in  two  categories  of  feeding  behavior:  ( 1 )  appetitive 
responses  (e.g..  head-up  feeding  posture  and  directed  head 
turning),  which  orient  the  animal  to  potential  goal  objects 
such  as  food;  and  (2)  consummatory  responses  (biting, 
swallowing),  which  obtain  the  goal  object.  The  consum- 
matory responses  are  rhythmic  and  relatively  stereotyped, 
whereas  the  appetitive  responses  are  highly  variable.  Our 
evidence  suggests  that  one  consummatory  response,  bit- 
ing, appears  to  be  controlled  by  command  elements  in 
the  cerebral-ganglion,  such  as  neuron  CBI-2,  which  are 
capable  of  driving  the  behavior.  One  component  of  the 
appetitive  behavior,  head  lifting,  may  be  controlled  (at 
least  in  part)  by  another  cerebral  neuron,  C-PR.  C-PR, 
however,  affects  numerous  systems  in  the  animal,  but  all 
the  systems  affected  seem  to  be  involved  in  the  food-in- 
duced arousal  state  of  the  animal.  We  postulate  that  C- 
PR  is,  in  some  ways,  analogous  to  command  neurons  that 
evoke  behaviors.  The  C-PR,  however,  not  only  evokes  a 
behavior,  but  also  evokes  a  central  motive  state  which 
aids  in  insuring  that  behavior  is  efficiently  expressed. 

Introduction 

Mollusks  have  long  been  used  for  studies  that  are  de- 
signed to  investigate  general  neurobiological  principles 
rather  than  the  details  of  a  single  species.  One  important 
advantage  of  mollusks  is  the  large  size  of  their  neurons. 

Received  7  August  1990;  accepted  6  November  1990. 


For  many  years,  studies  that  were  difficult  or  impossible 
in  vertebrates  could  be  approached  by  investigating  the 
squid  giant  axon  and  the  large  somata  of  gastropod  neu- 
rons. In  recent  years,  the  use  of  cell  culture,  brain  slices, 
and  other  methodologies  has  made  it  possible  to  do  many 
types  of  cellular  studies  on  vertebrate  neurons  that  could 
previously  be  done  only  in  mollusks.  However,  it  is  still 
very  difficult  to  study  the  integrative  functions  of  the  ver- 
tebrate nervous  system  and  to  relate  cellular  processes  to 
behavior.  For  this  reason,  the  presence  of  a  relatively  few 
neurons  in  gastropod  mollusks  has  assumed  increased 
importance. 

We  have  been  studying  the  marine  mollusk  Aplysia  in 
order  to  understand  the  neural  organization  and  the  cel- 
lular mechanisms  underlying  behavioral  states.  We  have 
concentrated  on  feeding  behavior  because  our  early  studies 
indicated  that  the  feeding  responses  of  these  animals  are 
modulated  by  a  number  of  interesting  state  variables,  in- 
cluding arousal  and  satiation  (Kupfermann  el  a/.,  1982; 
Susswein  et  ai,  1978).  This  paper  is  a  review  of  our  work. 
It  emphasizes  studies  of  the  appetitive  aspects  of  feeding, 
and  is  not  meant  to  be  a  general  review  of  feeding  in 
Aplysia. 

Feeding  Behavior  in  Aplysia  has  Similarities  to 
Motivated  Behaviors  in  Other  Animals 

To  provide  themselves  with  adequate  nutrients,  Aplysia 
has  many  of  the  same  problems  faced  by  most  other  an- 
imals. They  must  detect  and  locate  appropriate  food 
sources.  They  must  approach  the  food  and  orient  it  to  the 
buccal  orifice.  They  must  then  bite  and  swallow  the  food. 
Finally,  when  a  sufficient  amount  has  been  consumed, 
they  need  to  stop  feeding.  These  operations  must  all  be 
carried  out  in  a  manner  that  is  efficient  in  time  and  energy 
expenditure.  One  of  the  means  by  which  the  animals  im- 


262 


STUDIES  OF  BEHAVIORAL  STATE  IN  APLYSIA 


263 


\ 


Figure  1.     Aplysia  in  the  feeding  posture.  In  this  position  the  animal 
shows  directed  turning  responses  to  seaweed  applied  to  the  head. 


prove  the  efficiency  of  their  behavior  is  by  regulating  it 
according  to  particular  internal  states.  These  internal  states 
are  modulated  by  external  and  internal  stimuli  and  by  an 
internal  endogenous  process  associated  with  a  circadian 
activity  rhythm.  In  higher  animals,  the  constellation  of 
state  variables  that  regulate  feeding  are  termed  "hunger," 
and  by  analogy,  a  hunger-like  state  also  appears  to  regulate 
feeding  in  Aplysia.  As  in  higher  animals,  feeding  in  Aplysia 
is  greatly  potentiated  by  pre-exposing  the  animals  to  food; 
i.e..  the  animal  exhibits  incentive  motivation.  When  a 
quiescent  Aplysia  is  first  stimulated  with  seaweed,  it  be- 
comes activated  after  a  relatively  long  delay  (up  to  a  min- 


Figure  2.  Vectors  indicating  the  magnitude  and  direction  that  the 
head  turns  in  response  to  tactile  stimuli  briefly  presented  (open  loop)  to 
different  points  on  the  rhinophores  and  tentacles.  The  movements  turn 
the  head  in  the  direction  of  the  stimulus.  In  the  open  loop  condition  the 
animal  greatly  overshoots  the  stimulus.  If,  however,  the  stimulus  is 
maintained  in  place  (closed  loop),  when  the  animal  begins  the  response, 
the  movement  is  represented  by  the  indicated  vectors,  but  as  the  animal 
turns,  the  response  progressively  decreases  in  magnitude  so  that  the  mouth 
comes  to  be  accurately  centered  over  the  stimulus.  Data  from  Teyke 
el  al  (1990b) 


too- 

80- 

*\  °\i    Y  \i 
\  H  '    °\l 

,o 

60- 
40- 

\T 
'\I          * 

20- 
n  . 

?^\_,          °-° 

seaweed 
tactile 

01         23456789        10 
STIMULUS  NUMBER 

Figure  3.  Turning  angle  evoked  by  repeated  seaweed  (open  circles) 
or  tactile  (filled  circles)  stimuli.  The  animals  (n  =  5)  were  first  induced 
into  the  feeding  posture  by  means  of  seaweed.  They  were  then  stimulated 
at  a  locus  10°  from  the  mouth,  either  with  a  purely  tactile  stimulus,  or 
with  seaweed.  The  stimulus  was  repeated  every  10  s  ( 10  successive  stimuli; 
3  series  each).  Final  turning  angles  of  the  responses  are  shown  as  the 
percent  of  the  final  angle  of  the  first  response  (means  ±  S.E.M.).  Note 
the  marked  decline  in  the  magnitude  of  the  turning  response  evoked  by 
repeated  tactile  stimulation  and  the  relatively  steady  response  magnitude 
upon  repeated  seaweed  stimulation.  Data  from  Teyke  et  al.  (1990b). 


ute).  We  refer  to  this  activated  state  as  "food-induced 
arousal." 

Food-induced  arousal  in  Aplysia  manifests  itself  in  at 
least  two  stages.  First,  appetitive  behaviors  (the  orienting 
phase  of  motivated  behaviors)  are  affected;  second,  con- 
summatory  responses  are  modified.  Initial  contact  with 
food  evokes  a  defensive  withdrawal  reflex  of  the  head. 
The  fast  phase  of  this  reflex  appears  to  be  controlled  by 
the  cerebral  Bn  neurons  (Teyke  el  al,  1989),  which  receive 
powerful  tactile  input,  and  which  evoke  withdrawal 
movements  of  the  head  and  tentacles.  After  the  initial 
defensive  response,  the  animal  ceases  to  withdraw.  The 
response  appears  to  be  habituated,  but  unlike  other  forms 
of  habituation  in  Aplysia  (Castellucci  et  al..  1 970,  see  also 
Fig.  3),  the  response  decrement  occurs  very  rapidly,  typ- 
ically following  just  a  single  application  of  the  stimulus. 
A  subsequent  brief  food  stimulus  elicits  an  orienting  re- 
sponse, instead  of  eliciting  withdrawal.  The  animal  gets 
into  a  characteristic  feeding  posture  in  which  the  posterior 
part  of  the  foot  is  attached  to  the  substrate,  and  the  neck, 
head,  and  anterior  part  of  the  foot  are  lifted  (Fig.  1).  In 
addition,  there  are  signs  of  "autonomic"  arousal,  such  as 
an  increase  in  blood  pressure  and  heart  rate  (Koch  et  al.. 
1984).  The  feeding  posture  is  maintained  even  when  the 
food  is  removed,  indicating  that  the  appetitive  arousal 
has  a  "memory"  component.  From  the  feeding  posture 
the  animal  can  readily  move  its  head  toward  a  source  of 
food.  When  the  tentacles  of  the  food-aroused  animal  make 
physical  contact  with  food  (seaweed),  the  animal  moves 
its  head  so  as  to  direct  its  mouth  towards  the  stimulus 
(Fig.  2).  For  a  brief  (open  loop)  stimulus  within  the  re- 
ceptive field,  the  animal  greatly  overshoots  the  food,  and 
the  amount  of  overshoot  is  proportional  to  the  angular 


264 
BMN 

B16 


C12 


I.  KUPFERMANN  F.T  Al. 


U  ILL  IlILllliLL 
JJJIJJJJIIJUJU 


Figure  4.  Example  of  the  motor  program  dnven  by  CBI-2.  CBI-2  was  fired  by  a  constant  depolarizing 
current  (dark  horizontal  line).  The  rhythmic  program  incorporated  neurons  in  the  cerebral  and  buccal 
ganglia.  The  buccal  program  is  reflected  in  the  activity  of  an  identified  ARC  muscle  motor  neuron.  B16. 
Another  buccal  motor  neuron.  BMN.  illustrates  that  the  program  is  present  in  numerous  other  buccal 
neurons.  CI2  is  a  cerebral  ganglion  neuron  that  controls  movements  of  the  lips,  and  it  is  one  of  several 
cerebral  neurons  that  is  recruited  by  the  buccal  program  that  is  driven  by  CBI-2.  CBI-2  also  shows  periodic 
synaptic  input  driven  by  the  buccal  program.  Note  that  when  CBI-2  stops  firing,  the  program  briefly  persists 
and  then  terminates.  The  data  are  from  Rosen  el  al.  ( 1 987.  1 988). 


10  i 


JlO  mV 


distance  of  the  stimulus  from  the  mouth.  If,  however,  the 
stimulus  is  maintained  in  position  so  that  it  provides  con- 
tinuous feedback  during  the  movement  (closed  loop),  the 
food  is  accurately  centered  over  the  mouth  (Teyke  et  al., 
1990b).  Seaweed  provides  the  animal  with  two  distinct 
types  of  stimuli:  tactile  and  chemical.  Surprisingly,  the 
stimulus  that  results  in  the  animal  turning  toward  the 
food  is  the  tactile  component.  A  purely  chemical  stimulus, 
provided  by  an  aqueous  extract  of  seaweed,  is  not  very 
effective  in  eliciting  turning.  On  the  other  hand,  if  the 
animal  is  first  aroused  with  a  chemical  stimulus,  a  purely 
tactile  stimulus  (provided  by  a  glass  rod)  very  effectively 
evokes  a  turning  response.  If,  however,  the  tactile  stimulus 
is  repeated  without  intermittent  chemical  stimulation,  the 
turning  response  habituates  until  no  response  at  all  is 
evoked  (Fig.  3).  Thus,  the  chemical  component  of  the 
seaweed  maintains  the  arousal  level  of  the  animal,  while 
the  tactile  component  directs  the  response. 

When  the  animal  turns  toward  the  stimulus,  contact 
with  food  to  the  region  immediately  around  the  mouth 
(perioral  zone)  initiates  consummatory  behaviors  and  a 
new  set  of  arousal  responses.  The  consummatory  arousal 
is  characterized  by  a  progressive  build-up  of  the  rate  and 
magnitude  of  the  rhythmic  biting  response  that  occurs 
when  food  touches  the  perioral  zone  ( Kupfermann,  1974; 
Weiss  et  al.,  1982).  Whereas  the  appetitive  feeding  re- 
sponses are  highly  variable,  the  consummatory  biting  re- 
sponse is  more  stereotyped,  although  it  consists  of  several 
components  (Kupfermann,  1974;  Weiss  et  al..  1986):  (1) 
There  is  a  forward  movement  (cocking)  of  the  whole  buc- 


cal mass.  The  forward  position  is  maintained  during  the 
whole  meal.  (2)  The  whole  buccal  mass  undergoes  forward 
and  backward  movements.  These  movements  occur  on 
a  background  of  the  maintained  forward  movement.  (3) 
The  radula  rotates  forward  and  backward.  (4)  The  radula 
halves  open  and  close.  The  latter  two  movements  cause 
the  food  to  be  grasped  and  deposited  into  the  buccal  cavity. 
The  relatively  small  backward  movement,  which  deposits 
the  food  in  the  buccal  cavity  during  biting  behavior,  can 
be  distinguished  from  a  larger  backward  movement 
(swallowing)  that  is  triggered  by  the  presence  of  food  in 
the  buccal  cavity,  and  which  results  in  the  food  being 
moved  into  the  esophagus  (Kupfermann.  1974).  Biting 
movements,  which  are  elicited  by  food  contacting  the 
perioral  zone,  thus  consist  of  a  large  forward  component 
of  the  radula.  followed  by  a  relatively  small  backward 
movement.  Swallowing,  which  is  elicited  by  food  in  the 
buccal  cavity,  consists  of  a  relatively  small  forward  move- 
ment and  a  large  backward  movement.  The  swallowing 
movements  are  associated  with  an  inhibition  of  the  biting 
movements;  i.e..  as  long  as  food  is  present  in  the  buccal 
cavity,  stimulation  of  the  perioral  zone  never  elicits  a  large 
forward  movement  of  the  radula. 

Biting  Responses  are  Elicited  by  the  Activity  of 
Individual  Neurons  Located  in  the  Cerebral  Ganglion 

In  a  number  of  species,  including  gastropod  mollusks, 
stereotyped  responses  are  elicited  by  the  activity  of  indi- 
vidual cells  or  small  groups  of  cells  (Kupfermann  and 


STUDIES  OF  BEHAVIORAL  STATE  IN  APLYSIA 


265 


A         Postural   System 


B     Consummatory   System 


CBI-2 


MCC 


CPR 


CPR 


Cardiovascular   System  D       Defensive   Systems 


L10 


Bncell 


OPR 


CPR 


Figure  5.  The  various  effects  of  firing  C-PR  on  different  systems 
associated  with  food-induced  arousal.  For  each  experiment,  C-PR  was 
intracellularly  stimulated  at  20  Hz  for  5  s.  For  illustrative  purposes,  mul- 
tiple follower  cells  of  the  C-PR  are  shown  for  each  part  of  the  figure,  but 
the  data  for  each  trace  were  obtained  in  separate  experiments.  (A)  Ex- 
amples of  the  effects  of  firing  C-PR  on  different  pedal  ganglion  neurons, 
that  may  be  part  of  the  postural  control  system.  (B)  Effects  of  C-PR  on 
cerebral  ganglion  neurons  that  control  consummatory  feeding  responses 
(biting  command  element,  CBI-2,  and  the  modulatory  neuron,  meta- 
cerebral  cell,  MCC).  (C)  Effects  of  C-PR  on  abdominal  ganglion  neurons 
that  control  the  cardiovascular  system  (command  element  LIO.  heart 
exciter  RBHE,  and  vasoconstrictor  LBVC.  (D)  Effects  of  C-PR  on  various 
neurons  that  participate  in  defensive  responses  [head  withdrawal  neuron, 
Bn  cell  (Teyke  el  al..  1989);  gill  withdrawal  motor  neurons,  L7;  defensive 
secretion  neurons,  R2  and  PL1],  Calibration:  2  s,  20  mV,  except  5  mV 
for  cells  R2  and  PL1  in  part  D.  Data  are  from  Teyke  cl  al.  (1990a). 


Weiss,  1978;  Gillette  el  al,  1982;  McClellan,  1986;  Ben- 
jamin and  Elliott,  1989;  McCrohan  and  Kyriakides,  1989; 
Delaney  and  Gelperin,  1990).  To  determine  the  critical 
control  elements  for  the  consummatory  phase  of  feeding 
in  Aplyxia,  we  back-filled  the  cerebral-buccal  connectives 
and  located  a  population  of  cerebral  neurons  that  send 
their  axons  to  the  buccal  ganglion.  Several  of  these  cells 
had  been  previously  identified,  including  the  serotonergic 
metacerebral  cells  (MCCs)  and  ICBM  mechanosensory 
cells  (Rosen  et  al..  1989a,b).  In  addition,  two  small  pop- 
ulations of  cells  were  found  in  anterior  and  lateral  posi- 
tions. Firing  of  one  of  the  cells  (cerebral  to  buccal  inter- 
neuron  two,  or  CBI-2)  within  the  anterior  cluster,  pro- 


duced a  robust  and  reliable  rhythmic  motor  program  of 
the  buccal  ganglion  (Fig.  4)  (Rosen  el  al..  1987).  CBI-2 
receives  chemosensory  input  from  the  perioral  zone.  In 
addition,  when  it  elicits  a  buccal  motor  program  it  receives 
rhythmic  synaptic  input  from  the  buccal  ganglion,  and 
thus  it  fires  in  phase  with  the  buccal  motor  program.  If, 
however,  the  synaptic  feedback  from  the  buccal  ganglion 
is  blocked  by  placing  the  cerebral  ganglion  in  seawater 
containing  cobalt  ions,  the  firing  of  CBI-2  still  evokes 
rhythmic  activity  in  the  buccal  ganglion,  in  the  absence 
of  rhythmic  activity  in  CBI-2  (Rosen  et  al.,  1988).  For  a 
discussion  of  recent  work  on  the  central  pattern  generating 
circuitry  intrinsic  to  the  buccal  ganglion  of  Aplysia,  see 
Kirk  (1989),  Nagahama  and  Takata  (1989),  and  Susswein 
and  Byrne  (1988). 

Using  a  semi-intact  preparation,  we  found  that  the  firing 
of  CBI-2  can  evoke  rhythmic  movements  of  the  buccal 
mass  and  radula,  and  the  movements  are  similar  to  the 
repetitive  biting  responses  seen  in  the  intact  animal.  The 
responses  do  not  resemble  swallowing  or  rejection.  The 
firing  of  two  other  cerebral  to  buccal  interneurons  also 
evokes  coordinated  buccal  ganglion  activity,  but  the  motor 
programs  are  different  for  each  of  the  CBIs.  Thus  we  hy- 
pothesize that  the  CBIs  in  Aplysia,  as  in  other  gastropods 
(Gillette  et  al.,  1982;  Benjamin  and  Elliott,  1989;  Mc- 
Crohan and  Kyriakides,  1989;  Delaney  and  Gelperin, 
1990),  may  constitute  a  command  system,  the  conjoint 
activity  of  which  drives  consummatory  feeding  responses. 

Activity  of  an  Identified  Cerebral  Neuron  Appears  to 
Elicit  Elements  of  Appetitive  Arousal 

Although  stereotyped  consummatory  responses  are 
driven  by  a  relatively  few  command-like  elements,  it  is 
difficult  to  imagine  how  the  highly  variable  responses  that 
constitute  appetitive  behavior  could  be  similarly  driven 
by  a  small  number  of  neurons.  Nevertheless,  we  set  out 
to  determine  whether  the  nervous  system  contains  neu- 
rons that  can  evoke  appetitive  feeding  behavior.  Backfills 
of  the  cerebral-pedal  connectives  revealed  a  small  subset 
of  cerebral  ganglion  neurons  that  send  their  axons  to  the 
pedal  or  pleural  ganglia  (Teyke  et  al.,  1990a).  The  firing 
of  these  neurons  revealed  a  single  (bilateral)  cerebral  neu- 
ron that  can  influence  the  activity  of  numerous  neurons 
in  the  abdominal,  pedal,  and  cerebral  ganglia.  We  termed 
this  neuron  the  cerebral  to  pedal  regulator  [to  avoid  con- 
fusion with  the  caudal  photoreceptor  (CPR)  interneuron 
of  crayfish,  we  abbreviate  this  neuron  C-PR,  although 
previously  we  did  not  use  the  hyphen]. 

The  pedal  ganglion  in  particular  contains  a  large  num- 
ber of  neurons  that  are  excited  by  C-PR  (Fig.  5).  A  smaller 
number  of  pedal  ganglion  neurons  are  inhibited  by  C- 
PR.  Each  neuron  that  is  affected  by  C-PR  activity  receives 
input  following  the  firing  of  either  the  left  or  right  C-PR, 


266 


I.  KUPFERMANN  ET  AL 


OPR 


MCC 

Figure  6.     Example  of  a  prolonged  excitatory  response  in  C-PR  and  the  MCC  to  a  brief  seaweed  stimulus. 
Calibration:  5  s,  20  mV.  Data  from  Teyke  et  al.  (1990a). 


suggesting  that  C-PR  is  probably  not  directly  involved  in 
the  directed  head  turning  response,  which  is  very  strongly 
lateralized.  Nevertheless,  head  turning  does  not  occur  un- 
less the  animal  is  first  aroused,  so  that  C-PR  activity  may 
enable  head  turning.  Some  of  the  effects  of  C-PR  are 
monosynaptic,  whereas  others  are  mediated  by  interneu- 
rons.  Firing  of  many  of  the  pedal  cells  that  are  affected 
by  the  activity  of  C-PR  causes  the  muscles  of  the  anterior- 
dorsal  region  of  the  neck  to  contract  (Teyke  et  al..  1990a), 
which  suggests  that  C-PR  may  evoke  movements  that 
cause  the  head  to  be  lifted  into  the  feeding  posture.  Con- 
sistent with  a  role  of  C-PR  in  eliciting  head-lifting  in  re- 
sponse to  food,  we  found  that  seaweed  applied  to  the  ten- 
tacles evokes  strong  activity  in  C-PR  (Fig.  6).  Furthermore, 
preliminary  studies  involving  extracellular  recordings 
from  the  cerebral-pedal  connectives,  support  the  idea  that 
C-PR  is  active  just  before  and  during  the  time  that  the 


TIME  (SEC) 

Figure  7.  Sequence  of  mouth  openings  and  closings  during  seven 
repetitive  biting  responses,  in  two  animals  that  have  had  bilateral  chronic 
lesions  (protease  injected)  of  the  MCCs.  The  sequence  is  based  on  a 
videotaped  analysis  of  biting.  Width  of  the  mouth  opening  is  expressed 
as  a  percentage  of  the  maximum.  A.  An  example,  illustrating  "stuck" 
radula  in  a  MCC  lesioned  animal,  in  which  the  radula  stays  protracted 
for  an  abnormally  long  duration.  B.  Responses  of  a  normal  animal  (Bl ), 
and  of  the  same  animal  (B2)  following  lesion  of  the  MCCs.  Data  from 
Rosen  et  al.  (I989a). 


animal  lifts  its  head  into  the  feeding  posture  (Teyke  et  al., 
1990c).  Thus,  the  total  complex  of  appetitive  feeding  re- 
sponses may  consist  of  two  components:  a  stereotyped 
postural  head-lifting  response  and  a  more  varied  directed 
turning  response. 

We  have  formulated  a  simple  neural  model  (Teyke  et 
a/..  1990b)  whose  input-output  functions  are  similar  to 
the  behavioral  results  concerning  the  directed  head  turning 
component  of  appetitive  feeding  behavior.  The  model  is 
based  on  reflex  circuits  and  does  not  contain  command 
elements.  By  contrast,  the  head  lifting  response  may  be 
importantly  controlled  by  a  small  number  of  neurons, 
such  as  C-PR,  that  have  command-like  properties.  C-PR, 
however,  affects  responses  other  than  head  lifting.  In  fact, 


B 


Feeding  1 
Latency  1 

3osture 
sees) 

Ir 

ter-Bite  Interval 
(sees) 

30 

f^n  K 
&JL 

1CC            30 
esion 

- 

T  W 

m 

I  Cell 
.esion 

.A. 

20 

FT%  K 
i&JS 

ICC           2.0 
ihom 

•  •  •  • 

i  •  •• 

•  •  •< 
•  •  •  i 

10 

• 

1.0 

™ 

01 

zr 

tr&a 
y££j 
i 

-10 

LiJ    -I 

-1.0 

- 

JL 

-20 

-2.0 

_ 

Figure  8.  MCC  lesion  (n  =  6),  B  cell  control  lesion  (n  =  7),  and  dye 
injection  MCC  control  (n  =  6)  group  mean  difference  scores  (postop- 
erative overall  mean,  minus  preoperative  scores,  ±  SEMs)  for  latency  to 
assume  feeding  posture  (A)  and  for  interbite  interval  (B).  Bites  were 
elicited  by  continually  stimulating  the  lips  with  seaweed,  without  allowing 
the  animal  to  obtain  the  food.  Data  from  Rosen  et  al.  (1989a). 


STUDIES  OF  BEHAVIORAL  STATE  IN  APLYSIA 


267 


o 

LJ 
l/l 


a: 

UJ 


20-- 


15-- 


3      10 
O 


5-- 


D 


MCC  Lesion,  Pre-Op 
*        •       MCC  Lesion,  Post-Op 

D D       Control,  Pre-Op 

Q Q       Control,  Post-Op 


012345678910111213 
SWALLOW  NUMBER 

Figure  9.  MCC  lesion  and  control  groups  mean  interswallow  inter- 
vals, measured  dunng  ingestion  of  strips  of  seaweed.  No  significant  group 
differences  were  found.  Data  from  Rosen  ct  ill  ( 1989a). 


we  found  that  C-PR  activity  affects  neurons  involved  in 
three  other  types  of  responses:  defensive  withdrawal  re- 
flexes (Fig.  5D),  consummatory  biting  (Fig.  5B),  and  car- 
diovascular responses  (Fig.  5C).  The  neurons  involved  in 
defensive  responses  were  inhibited  by  the  firing  of  C-PR. 
and  in  semi-intact  preparations  we  showed  that  the  firing 
of  head  withdrawal  neurons  in  response  to  a  strong  tactile 
stimulus  to  the  head  was  reduced  when  the  C-PR  neuron 
was  permitted  to  fire.  The  rapid  depression  of  withdrawal 
responses  following  contact  with  seaweed  may  therefore 
be  due  either  in  part,  or  wholly,  to  an  active  inhibition, 
rather  than  to  low  frequency  depression,  as  appears  to  be 
the  case  for  habituation  of  the  gill  and  siphon  reflex  to 
tactile  stimulation  (Castellucci  el  al,  1970). 

Firing  of  C-PR  evokes  complex  mixtures  of  excitatory 
and  inhibitory  synaptic  responses  in  abdominal  ganglion 
neurons  controlling  the  heart  and  blood  vessels  (Fig.  5C). 
These  effects  could  contribute  to  aspects  of  cardiovascular 
responses  that  occur  during  food-induced  arousal. 

The  cerebral  ganglion  neurons  involved  in  consum- 
matory behaviors  generally  receive  pure  excitation  when 
C-PR  is  fired.  These  neurons  include  command-like  ele- 
ments for  biting  (CBI-2)  and  the  metacerebral  cells 
(MCCs).  The  MCCs  modulate  the  muscles  and  neurons 
that  effectuate  biting  and  account,  in  part,  for  the  build- 
up of  the  speed  and  magnitude  of  successive  bites,  which 
occurs  during  consummatory  arousal  (Rosen  el  ai, 
1989a).  In  contrast  to  the  C-PR,  the  modulatory  effects 
of  the  MCC  are  very  restricted.  It  only  modulates  con- 
summatory responses,  and  only  the  bite  component.  For 
example,  if  the  MCCs  are  destroyed,  there  is  no  change 
in  the  capacity  to  elicit  the  feeding  posture  (Fig.  8)  (Rosen 
et  al.,  1989a),  but  there  is  an  increase  in  the  bite  latency 
and  inter-bite  intervals  (Fig.  7,  8B).  Inter-swallow  intervals 
are  unchanged  (Fig.  9). 


When  C-PR  is  fired  at  physiological  rates,  its  excitatory 
effect  on  neurons  involved  in  biting  responses  is  never 
strong  enough  to  drive  the  neurons  at  a  rate  sufficient  to 
evoke  biting.  C-PR  appears  to  function  to  increase  the 
excitability  of  these  neurons  without  directly  driving  con- 
summatory responses. 

By  cutting  various  connectives  we  could  localize  the 
ganglia  that  contain  the  interneurons  that  produce  the 
effects  of  C-PR  on  the  various  non-postural  systems.  We 
found  that  all  of  these  effects  are  mediated  by  the  activity 
of  the  pedal-pleural  ganglia.  It  may  be  significant  that 
these  ganglia  mediate  the  postural  responses  associated 
with  food  arousal.  Thus  appetitive  arousal  may  involve  a 
primary  effect  on  a  postural  system,  which,  in  turn,  mod- 
ulates the  activity  of  the  numerous  other  systems  that  will 
eventually  come  into  play  during  feeding.  In  the  vertebrate 
brain,  indeed,  neurons  thought  to  be  concerned  with  reg- 
ulation of  consciousness  and  arousal  are  concentrated  in 
the  brain  stem  in  regions  intimately  involved  with  postural 
regulation  (Hobson  and  Brazier,  1980).  Because  virtually 
all  behaviors  require  a  particular  posture  for  their  exe- 
cution, the  postural  neural  system  may  serve  a  primary 
role  in  arousal  in  highly  diverse  species. 

Some  of  the  effects  of  C-PR,  such  as  those  on  the  ele- 
ments of  consummatory  responses,  could  enhance  these 
responses.  Other  effects,  such  as  those  on  Bn  neurons, 
may  suppress  responses  that  are  incompatible  with  feeding 
behavior.  We  postulate  that  C-PR  is,  in  some  ways,  anal- 
ogous to  command  neurons,  which  evoke  behaviors.  The 
C-PR,  however,  not  only  evokes  a  behavior  (head  lifting), 
but  also  evokes  a  central  motive  state  that  aids  in  insuring 
that  behavior  is  efficiently  expressed.  A  behavioral  action 
such  as  feeding  is  made  up  of  a  number  of  different  be- 
havioral acts  (e.g.,  head  lifting,  biting,  swallowing).  Thus, 
a  consideration  of  the  ways  in  which  behavioral  efficiency 
is  maximized  raises  two  fundamental  questions.  First,  how 
are  multiple  responses  of  the  organism  coordinated  with 
one  another,  and  second,  how  are  the  individual  behav- 
ioral acts  which  make  up  a  behavioral  action  modulated 
so  as  to  optimize  their  speed  and  minimize  energy  ex- 
penditure? Our  evidence  suggests  that  one  means  of  co- 
ordinating diverse  responses  directed  toward  a  single  goal 
is  to  affect  diverse  neuronal  systems  through  the  activity 
of  a  relatively  few  neuronal  elements.  Data  presented 
elsewhere  indicate  that  maximization  of  the  efficiency  of 
individual  responses  is  accomplished,  in  part,  by  the  ac- 
tivity of  subordinate  specialized  neurons  such  as  the  MCC 
(Weiss  et  al..  1978;  Rosen  et  al..  1989a).  In  addition,  in- 
dividual responses  may  be  regulated  by  neuromodulators 
that  occur  as  cotransmitters  in  motorneurons  innervating 
the  muscles  that  effectuate  feeding  responses  (Lloyd  et  a!., 
1985;  Cropper  et  al.,  1987a,b,  1988,  1990).  The  motor 
neurons  are  subordinate  to  the  modulatory  effects  of  the 
MCC,  which,  in  turn,  is  modulated  by  C-PR.  Thus  the 


268 


I    KUPFERMANN  ET  AL. 


final  motor  activity  appears  to  be  regulated  by  modulatory 
neurons  of  progressively  higher  order.  We  are  beginning 
to  reduce  the  elusive  concept  of  motivational  state  to  ex- 
planations in  terms  of  the  actions  of  ordinary  neural 
mechanisms,  operating  in  networks  of  appropriately  in- 
terconnected neurons. 

Literature  Cited 

Benjamin,  P.  R.,  and  C.  J.  H.  Elliott.  1989.  Snail  feeding  oscillator: 
the  central  pattern  generator  and  its  control  by  modulatory  inter- 
neurons.  Pp.  173-214  in  Neuronal  and  Cellular  Oscillators.  J.  W. 
Jacklet,  ed.  Marcel  Dekker.  Inc.,  New  York  and  Basel. 

Castellucci,  V.,  H.  Pinsker,  I.  Kupfermann,  and  E.  R.  kandel. 
1970.  Neuronal  mechanisms  of  habituation  and  dishabituation  of 
the  gill-withdrawal  reflex  in  Aplysia.  Science  167:  1745-1748. 

Cropper,  E.  C.,  R.  Tenenbaum,  M.  A.  Gawinowicz  Kolks,  I.  Kupfermann, 
and  K.  R.  Weiss.  1987a.  Myomodulin:  a  bioactive  neuropeptide 
present  in  an  identified  cholinergic  buccal  motor  neuron  ofAplysia. 
Proc.  Nat/.  Acad.  Sci.  USA  84:  5483-5486. 

Cropper,  E.  C.,  P.  E.  Lloyd,  VV.  Reed,  R.  Tenenbaum,  I.  Kupfermann, 
and  K.  R.  Weiss.  1987b.  Multiple  neuropeptides  in  cholinergic 
motor  neurons  of  Aplysia:  evidence  for  modulation  intrinsic  to  the 
motor  circuit.  Proc.  Nail  Acad.  Sci.  USA  84:  3486-3490. 

Cropper,  E.  C.,  M.  W.  Miller,  R.  Tenenbaum,  M.  A.  Gawinowicz  Kolks, 
I.  Kupfermann,  and  K.  R.  Weiss.  1988.  Structure  and  action  of 
buccalin:  a  modulatory  neuropeptide  localized  to  an  identified  small 
cardioactive  peptide-containing  cholinergic  motor  neuron  ofAplysia 
californica.  Proc.  Nail.  Acad.  Sci.  USA  85:  6 1 77-6 181. 

Cropper,  E.  C.,  M.  W.  Miller,  F.  S.  Vilim,  R.  Tenenbaum,  I.  Kupfermann, 
and  K.  R.  Weiss.  1990.  Buccalin  is  present  in  the  cholinergic  motor 
neuron  B16  ofAplysia  and  it  depresses  accessory  radula  closer  muscle 
contractions  evoked  by  stimulation  of  B16.  Brain  Res  512:  175- 
179. 

Delaney,  K.,  and  A.  Gelperin.  1990.  Cerebral  interneurons  controlling 
fictive  feeding  in  Umax  maximus.  I.  Anatomy  and  criteria  for  re- 
identification.  J.  Comp.  Physiol.  [A]  166:  297-310. 

Gillette,  R.,  M.  P.  Kovac,  and  VV.  J.  Davis.  1982.  Control  of  feeding 
motor  output  by  paracerebral  neurons  in  brain  of  Pleurobranchaea 
californica.  J  Neurophysiol.  47:  885-908. 

Hobson,  J.  A.,  and  M.  A.  B.  Brazier,  eds.  1980.  The  Reticular  For- 
mation Revisited.  Specifying  Functionjbr  a  Nonspecific  Svstein.  Raven 
Press,  New  York.  552  pp. 

Kirk,  M.  D.  1989.  Premotor  neurons  in  the  feeding  system  of  Aplvsia 
californica.  J.  Neurobiol.  20:  497-512. 

Koch,  U.  T.,  J.  Koester,  and  K.  R.  Weiss.  1984.  Neuronal  mediation 
of  cardiovascular  effects  of  food  arousal  in  Aplvsia.  J  Neurophvsiol. 
51:  126-135. 

Kupfermann,  I.  1974.  Feeding  behavior  in  Aplysia:  a  simple  system  for 
the  study  of  motivation.  Behav.  Biol.  10:  1-26. 

Kupfermann,  I.,  L.  Shkolnik,  and  K.  R.  Weiss.  1982.  Modulatory  syn- 
aptic  actions  of  neurotransmitters  in  circuits  controlling  feeding  in 
Aplysia.  Pp.  517-531  in  Cytochemical  Methods  in  Neuroanatomy. 
Alan  R.  Liss.  Inc.,  New  York. 

Kupfermann,  I.,  and  K.  R.  Weiss.  1978.  The  command  neuron  concept. 
Behav.  Brain  Sci.  1:  3-10. 

Lloyd,  P.  E.,  A.  C.  Mahon,  I.  Kupfermann,  J.  L.  Cohen,  R.  H.  Scheller, 
and  K.  R.  Weiss.  1985.  Biochemical  and  immunocytological  lo- 


calization of  molluscan  small  cardioactive  peptides  in  the  nervous 
system  ofAplysia  californica.  J.  Neurosci.  5:  1851-1861. 

McClellan,  A.  D.  1986.  Command  systems  for  initiating  locomotion 
in  fish  and  amphibians:  parallels  to  initiation  systems  in  mammals. 
Pp.  3-20  in  Neurohiology  of  I  'ertehrale  Locomotion.  S.  Grillner,  R. 
Herman,  P.  Stein,  D.  Stuart,  eds.  MacMillan  Press,  London. 

McCrohan,  C.  R.,  and  M.  A.  Kyriakides.  1989.  Cerebral  interneurones 
controlling  feeding  motor  output  in  the  snail  Lvmnaea  stagnalis.  J. 
Exp.  Biol.  147:  361-374. 

Nagahama,  T.,  and  M.  Takata.  1990.  Neural  mechanism  generating 
firing  patterns  in  jaw  motoneurons  during  the  food-induced  response 
in  Aplysia  kurodai  II.  Functional  role  of  premotor  neurons  on  gen- 
eration of  firing  patterns  in  motoneurons. ./.  Comp.  Plivsiol.  [A]  166: 
277-286. 

Rosen,   S.   C.,   M.    W.    Miller,   K.   R.   Weiss,   and    I.   Kupfermann. 

1987.  Control  of  buccal  motor  programs  in  Aplysia  by  identified 
neurons  in  the  cerebral  ganglion.  Soc.  Neurosci.  Abs.  13:  1060. 

Rosen,   S.   C.,   M.    W.    Miller,    K.    R.   Weiss,   and   I.   Kupfermann. 

1988.  Activity  of  CBI-2  ofAplysia  elicits  biting-like  responses.  Soc. 
Neurosci.  Abs.  14:  608. 

Rosen,  S.  C.,  K.  R.  Weiss,  R.  S.  Goldstein,  and  I.  Kupfermann. 
1989a.  The  role  of  a  modulatory  neuron  in  feeding  and  satiation 
in  Aplysia:  effects  of  lesioning  of  the  serotonergic  metacerebral  cells. 
/  Neurosci.  9:  1562-1578. 

Rosen,  S.  C.,  A.  J.  Susswein,  E.  C.  Cropper,  K.  R.  Weiss,  and  I.  Kup- 
fermann. 1989b.  Selective  modulation  of  spike  duration  by  serotonin 
and  the  neuropeptides.  FMRFamide.  SCPB.  buccalin  and  myomod- 
ulin  in  different  classes  of  mechanoafferent  neurons  in  the  cerebral 
ganglion  ofAplysia.  J.  Neurosci.  9:  390-402. 

Susswein,  A.  J.,  K.  R.  Weiss,  and  I.  Kupfermann.  1978.  The  effects  of 
food  arousal  on  the  latency  of  biting  in  Aplysia.  J.  Comp.  Psvchol. 
123:  31-41. 

Susswein,  A.  J.,  and  J.  H.  Byrne.  1988.  Identification  and  character- 
ization of  neurons  initiating  patterned  neural  activity  in  the  buccal 
ganglion  ofAplysia.  J.  Neurosci.  8:  2049-2061. 

Teyke,  T.,  K.  R.  Weiss,  and  I.  Kupfermann.  1989.  A  subpopulation  of 
cerebral  B  cluster  neurones  ofAplysia  californica  is  involved  in  de- 
fensive head  withdrawal  but  not  appetitive  head  movements.  J.  Exp. 
Biol.  147:  1-20. 

Teyke,  T.,  K.  R.  Weiss,  and  1.  Kupfermann.  1990a.  In  vivo  firing  pattern 
of  the  food  arousal  neuron  CPR  in  Aplvsia.  Soc.  Neurosci.  Abs.  16: 
627. 

Teyke,  T.,  K.  R.  Weiss,  and  I.  Kupfermann.  1990b.  Appetitive  feeding 
behavior  ofAplysia:  behavioral  and  neural  analysis  of  directed  head 
turning.  J.  Neurosci.  10:  3922-3934. 

Teyke,  T.,  K.  R.  Weiss,  and  I.  Kupfermann.  1990c.  An  identified  neuron 
(CPR)  evokes  neuronal  responses  reflecting  food  arousal  in  Aplysia. 
Science  247:  85-87. 

Weiss,  K.  R.,  J.  L.  Cohen,  and  I.  Kupfermann.  1978.  Modulatory  con- 
trol of  buccal  musculature  by  a  serotonergic  neuron  (metacerebral 
cell)  in  Aplysia.  J  Neurophysiol.  41:  181-203. 

Weiss,  K.  R.,  II.  T.  Koch,  J.  Koester,  S.  C.  Rosen,  and  I.  Kupfermann. 
1982.  The  role  of  arousal  in  modulating  feeding  behavior  in  Aplysia: 
neural  and  behavioral  studies.  Pp.  25-57  in  The  Neural  Basis  of 
Feeding  and  Reward.  B.  G.  Hoebel.  and  D.  Novin.  eds.  Haer  Institute. 
Brunswick,  ME. 

Weiss,  K.  R.,  H.  J.  Chiel,  U.  Koch,  and  I.  Kupfermann.  1986.  Activity 
of  an  identified  histaminergic  neuron,  and  its  possible  role  in  arousal 
of  feeding  behavior  in  semi-intact  Aplysia.  J  Neurosci.  6:  2403-2415. 


Reference:  Bin!  Bull  180:  269-275.  (April, 


A  Comparison  of  Bursting  Neurons  in  Aplysia 

A.  ALEVIZOS1  \  M.  SKELTON1,  K.  R.  WEISS1-,  AND  J.  KOESTER1 2 

1 ]  Center  for  Newobiology  and  Behavior,  - Department  of  Psychiatry,  and  '  Department  of  Physiology 

and  Cellular  Biophysics,  College  of  Physicians  and  Surgeons,  Columbia  University. 

'l22  \V.  16S  St..  New  York.  New  York  10032 


Abstract.  Five  types  of  bursting  neurons  have  been  de- 
scribed in  Aplysia:  three  types  of  individual  bursters — the 
LUQ  cells,  L10,  and  R15,  plus  two  types  of  population 
bursters — the  bag  cells  and  the  R25/L25  cells.  Individual 
bursters  can  burst  without  any  synaptic  input,  while  bursts 
generated  by  the  population  bursters  are  shaped  largely 
by  their  synaptic  interactions.  In  this  paper  we  review 
what  is  known  about  the  burst  mechanisms  of  these  five 
classes  of  neurons  and  attempt  to  relate  them  to  the  roles 
of  the  five  cell  types  in  the  control  of  autonomic  function. 

Introduction 

Molluscan  neurons  that  have  endogenous  burst  gen- 
erating capabilities  are  useful  experimental  preparations 
for  the  study  of  burst  generation  and  modulation.  This  is 
particularly  true  for  Aplysia.  Because  the  neural  circuitry 
of  Aplysia  has  been  studied  extensively,  one  can  attempt 
to  relate  the  functional  properties  of  bursting  neurons  to 
their  roles  in  the  control  of  behavior,  and  to  begin  a  com- 
parative study  of  different  types  of  bursters  within  the 
same  organism.  In  this  paper  we  will  compare  and  contrast 
the  burst  mechanisms  and  functional  roles  of  five  different 
types  of  bursting  neurons,  all  of  which  are  found  in  the 
abdominal  ganglion,  and  all  of  which  have  unique  prop- 
erties: the  bag  cells,  the  R25/L25  cells,  cell  L10,  the  LUQ 
cells,  and  cell  R15.  Particular  attention  is  given  to  R15, 
which  has  been  the  focus  of  our  recent  studies.  The  in- 
teractions between  these  five  classes  of  bursting  cells,  as 
well  as  some  of  their  outputs,  are  shown  schematically  in 
Figure  1. 

The  Bag  Cells 

The  neuroendocrine  bag  cells  consist  of  two  symmet- 
rical clusters  of  about  400  neurosecretory  cells  each,  which 

Received  22  August  1990;  accepted  6  November  1990. 


are  strongly  coupled  to  one  another  electrically.  They  have 
only  one  mode  of  firing — a  synchronous  population  burst 
in  all  800  cells  that  lasts  approximately  1 5-30  min  (Kup- 
fermann  and  Kandel,  1970).  This  burst  is  necessary  and 
sufficient  to  trigger  normal  egg  laying  behavior  (Pinsker 
and  Dudek,  1977;  Dudek  et  a/..  1979).  Oviposition  is  a 
complex,  stereotyped  behavior  that  lasts  from  one  to  a 
few  hours  and  typically  occurs  at  an  interval  of  one  or 
more  days  (Cobbs  and  Pinsker,  1982;  Ferguson  et  al., 
1989).  The  physiological  stimulus  that  triggers  a  popu- 
lation burst  in  the  bag  cells  is  unknown,  but  when  the  bag 
cells  are  excited  experimentally,  the  intraburst  firing  fre- 
quency and  the  duration  of  the  burst  are  independent  of 
the  intensity  of  the  triggering  stimulus  (Kupfermann  and 
Kandel,  1970).  This  all-or-none  burst  triggers  the  release 
of  a  dose  of  egg-laying  hormone  into  the  circulatory  sys- 
tem. This  hormone  then  initiates  the  release  of  mature 
oocytes  from  the  ovotestis  (Dudek  et  al..  1980;  Rothman 
et  al..  1983b).  The  population  burst  has  an  exceptionally 
long  refractory  period,  lasting  on  the  order  of  18-24 
h.  which  limits  the  rate  of  occurrence  of  egg  laying 
(Kaczmarek  and  Kauer,  1983). 

The  all-or-none  nature  of  the  bag  cell  burst  results  from 
a  positive  feedback,  reverberatory  interaction  within  the 
population.  In  addition  to  releasing  egg  laying  hormone, 
the  bag  cells  also  release  three  neuropeptides,  a-,  £-,  and 
7-bag  cell  peptides,  which  are  autoexcitatory  (Rothman 
et  al..  1983a;  Brown  and  Mayeri,  1989).  Even  weak  ex- 
citation of  the  bag  cells  can  lead  to  an  all-or-none  burst, 
as  the  cells  excite  one-another  by  these  slow,  chemically 
mediated  interactions.  Not  only  do  these  peptides  depo- 
larize the  bag  cells,  they  also  down-regulate  voltage-sen- 
sitive K+  channels  and  up-regulate  Ca++  channels,  ren- 
dering the  cells  more  excitable  and  resulting  in  enhanced 
spike  duration.  These  effects  contribute  to  prolonging  the 
burst,  thus  ensuring  that  a  suprathreshold  dose  of  egg- 


269 


270 


A.  ALEVIZOS  ET  AL 


Figure  1.  Summary  of  the  interactions  between  bursting  cells  in  the 
abdominal  ganglion  ofAplysia.  The  dashed  lines  represent  indirect  con- 
nections. The  bag  cells.  L10,  and  the  R25/L25  cells  are  known  to  make 
several  other  connections  to  neurons  that  are  not  shown  here  for  simplicity 
(Koester  and  Kandel,  1977;  Mayen  and  Rothman.  1985;  Segal  and 
Koester.  1982). 


laying  hormone  is  released  into  the  blood.  These  effects 
on  ion  channels  are  apparently  mediated  in  part  by  ac- 
tivation of  A-kinase  and  C-kinase  (Strong  and  Kaczmarek, 
1986;  Strong  et  al,  1987;  De  Riemer  et  ai.  1985;  Conn 
et  al.,  1988).  An  initial  decrease  in  the  levels  of  cyclic 
AMP,  followed  at  a  longer  latency  by  a  decrease  in  sen- 
sitivity to  cyclic  AMP,  contribute  to  termination  of  the 
burst  as  well  as  to  the  refractory  period  that  follows  the 
burst  (Kauer  and  Kaczmarek,  1985). 

The  R25/L25  Neurons 

The  R25  and  L25  cells  are  two  interconnected  clusters 
of  approximately  15  cells  each,  which  act  as  trigger  cells 
for  respiratory  pumping.  They  connect  directly  to  the 
motoneurons  that  drive  the  behavior.  A  population  burst 
in  the  R25/L25  network  is  necessary  and  probably  suffi- 
cient for  triggering  the  complete  behavior  (Byrne,  1983; 
Koester,  1989).  Like  egg  laying,  respiratory  pumping  often 
occurs  episodically  and  in  an  all-or-none  fashion  (Pinsker 
etal..  1 970;  Eberly  and  Pinsker,  1984).  Unlike  egg  laying, 
each  episode  of  respiratory  pumping  is  brief,  consisting 
of  synchronous  contractions  of  the  mantle  organs  accom- 
panied by  heart  inhibition  (Pinsker  et  al..  1970;  Byrne 
and  Koester,  1978).  The  motor  effects  typically  last  only 
5-10  s.  Individual  episodes  can  occur  spontaneously  or 
in  response  to  tactile  or  noxious  stimuli  (Pinsker  et  al., 
1970;  Walters  and  Erickson,  1986).  Respiratory  pumping 
can  also  occur  repetitively — either  in  a  stationary  rhythm 
with  a  period  of  a  few  minutes  (unpub.  obs.)  or  in  a  de- 
celerating "seizure"  pattern  (Kanz  and  Quast,  1990). 
These  repetitive  episodes  of  respiratory  pumping  can  oc- 


cur spontaneously  or  in  response  to  various  environmental 
stimuli  (Eberly  et  al..  1981;Croll,  1985;  Kanz  and  Quast. 
1990).  The  functional  significance  of  respiratory  pumping 
appears  to  vary  with  the  context  in  which  it  occurs.  It  has 
been  hypothesized  that  respiratory  pumping  may  function 
to  enhance  defensive  withdrawal  (Pinsker  et  al.,  1970),  to 
expel  defensive  secretions  or  debris  from  the  mantle  cavity 
(Kupfermann  and  Kandel,  1969),  to  increase  respiratory 
exchange  (Byrne  and  Koester,  1978),  or  to  contribute  to 
the  systemic  circulation  of  hormones  (Kanz  and  Quast. 
1990). 

The  basic  mechanism  of  burst  generation  in  the  R25/ 
L25  network  resembles  that  of  the  bag  cells.  Low  fre- 
quency firing  leads  to  a  regenerative,  all-or-none  stereo- 
typed burst  that  results  from  positive  feedback  interactions 
between  cells  in  the  R25/L25  network.  Conventional  fa- 
cilitating chemical  EPSPs,  as  well  as  electrical  coupling, 
mediate  these  mutually  excitatory  connections.  This  pos- 
itive feedback  state  can  be  accessed  by  two  separate  path- 
ways— slow  pacemaker  potentials  that  are  endogenous  to 
the  R25/L25  cells  or  excitatory  chemical  EPSPs  that  are 
generated  by  afferent  input.  Termination  of  the  all-or- 
none  population  burst  in  these  cells  is  mediated  largely 
by  synaptic  interactions — slowly  developing  mutual  syn- 
aptic  inhibition  and  heterosynaptic  depression  of  the  mu- 
tually excitatory  chemical  connections  (Byrne,  1983; 
Koester,  1989). 

The  LUQ  Neurons 

The  left  upper  quadrant  (LUQ)  cells  are  a  cluster  of 
five  similar  neurons  (Frazier  et  ai,  1967).  A  subset  of  the 
LUQ  cells  project  to  the  kidney,  where  they  ramify  ex- 
tensively. On  the  basis  of  their  axonal  projections  they 
are  thought  to  have  extensive  effects  on  kidney  function. 
The  only  effects  of  these  cells  that  have  been  examined 
in  detail  are  on  the  renal  pore,  which  they  cause  to  close. 
The  synaptic  actions  of  the  LUQ  cells  on  this  pore  have 
very  slow  onsets  and  offsets,  on  the  order  of  several  seconds 
(Koester  and  Alevizos,  1989). 

Unlike  the  bag  cells  and  the  R25/L25  cells,  individual 
LUQ  cells  burst  independently  of  one  another.  Their  en- 
dogenous burst  properties  have  been  analyzed  in  detail 
(Kramer  and  Zucker,  1985a,b;  Thompson  et  al.,  1986). 
The  depolarizing  pacemaker  potential  of  each  burst  is  ini- 
tiated by  the  activation  of  voltage-dependent  Ca++  chan- 
nels. When  the  cell  reaches  action  potential  threshold,  the 
Ca++  influx  during  each  action  potential  causes  a  buildup 
of  cytoplasmic  free  Ca++,  which  has  three  effects.  The 
initial  effect  is  to  activate  Ca++-dependent,  non-specific 
cation-selective  channels,  which  contribute  to  burst  ac- 
celeration. Eventually  the  two  slower  effects  of  intracel- 
lular  Ca*  +-buildup  predominate:  (1)  Ca++-dependent  in- 
activation  of  the  Ca++  channels  that  initiated  the  depo- 


BURSTING  NEURONS  IN  APLYSIA 


271 


larizing  pacemaker  potential  leads  to  a  phase  of 
regenerative  repolarization.  (2)  Activation  of  Catf-de- 
pendent  K*  channels  also  contributes  to  the  repolariza- 
tion, particularly  at  low  temperatures. 

Neuron  L10 

L 10  also  bursts  endogenously  (Kandel,  1976;  Kleinfeld 
el  ai,  1990).  It  is  a  multiaction  interneuron  and  moto- 
neuron  that  is  thought  to  play  a  major  role  in  integrating 
various  aspects  of  renal  function.  It  makes  direct  and  in- 
direct connections  to  the  renal  pore  that  oppose  the  syn- 
aptic  actions  of  the  LUQ  cells — i.e.,  it  causes  the  pore  to 
open.  In  vitro  these  openings  occur  at  a  rate  of  about  one 
per  minute.  This  peripheral  antagonism  is  complemented 
by  direct  inhibitory  projections  from  L10  to  the  LUQ 
cells.  L10  also  ramifies  extensively  in  the  kidney  and  is 
presumed  to  modulate  other  aspects  of  renal  function 
(Koesterand  Alevizos,  1989).  One  way  in  which  L10  may 
modulate  renal  excretion  is  by  its  excitatory  connection 
to  the  heart  excitatory  motoneuron  RBHt  (Koester  et  al, 
1974).  The  bulk  filtration  that  gives  rise  to  renal  fluid  is 
thought  to  occur  within  a  specialized  structure,  the  cristae 
aorta,  which  lies  in  series  with  the  heart  in  the  pericardia! 
sac  (Andrews,  1988).  Therefore  the  increase  in  heart  rate 
caused  indirectly  by  L10  activity  may  increase  renal  fil- 
tration. 

The  mechanism  that  underlies  spontaneous  bursting 
in  L10  has  not  been  studied  in  detail.  However,  prelim- 
inary results  suggest  that  many  of  the  spikes  that  occur 
during  a  spontaneous  burst  are  generated  in  peripheral 
axonal  processes,  far  outside  the  ganglion  (unpub.  obs.). 

Neuron  R15 

R15  is  an  endogenously  bursting  peptidergic  neuron 
that  is  thought  to  play  a  role  in  integrating  various  aspects 
of  egg  laying.  It  was  observed  several  years  ago  that  spon- 
taneous burst  generation  by  R15  is  enhanced  by  the  bag 
cells  when  they  fire  in  their  population  burst  (Branton  et 
al..  1978).  More  recently,  using  an  in  vitro  preparation, 
it  has  been  found  that  R15  has  several  synaptic  actions 
that  may  contribute  to  efficient  egg  laying  behavior.  ( 1 ) 
When  R15  bursts  spontaneously,  it  increases  the  fre- 
quency of  respiratory  pumping  via  its  excitatory  connec- 
tions to  the  R25/L25  cells  (Alevizos  etal..  1991a).(2)R15 
causes  contraction  of  the  pleuroabdominal  connectives 
by  its  excitatory  connection  to  motoneuron  L7  (Alevizos 
et  ai,  1991b).  (3)  R15  increases  the  rate  of  anterograde 
peristalsis  of  the  large  hermaphroditic  duct  via  its  periph- 
eral axonal  processes  ( Alevizos  et  al.,  199  Ic).  (4)  R15  also 
sends  processes  to  the  left  pedal-parapodial  artery,  by 
which  it  causes  local  vasoconstriction  of  this  branch  of 
the  arterial  tree  (Skelton,  in  prep.). 


It  has  been  postulated  that  R 1 5  integrates  five  different 
aspects  of  egg  laying  behavior:  (1)  The  increase  in  respi- 
ratory pumping  rate  may  enhance  respiratory  exchange 
(Alevizos  et  ai,  199 la).  Alternatively,  the  vigorous  pres- 
sure surges  that  occur  in  the  arterial  system  as  the  result 
of  gill  contractions  may  assist  in  circulating  egg  laying 
hormone  throughout  the  body  (Kanz  and  Quast,  1990). 
(2)  L7  is  a  multiaction  excitatory  neuron  that  connects 
to  muscle  in  a  variety  of  organs,  as  well  as  to  neurons  in 
the  peripheral  nervous  system  (reviewed  by  Umitsu  et  al., 
1987;  Alevizos  et  ai,  1989).  At  the  low  rates  of  L7  firing 
elicited  by  R15  bursting  in  vitro,  the  only  synaptic  action 
that  L7  expresses  is  excitation  of  the  sheath  muscle  of  the 
paired  pleuroabdominal  connectives.  Each  connective 
consists  of  a  central  axonal  core  surrounded  by  a  con- 
nective tissue  sheath  that  contains  vascular  channels  into 
which  the  bag  cells  release  their  peptides  and  hormones. 
The  accordion-like  folding  of  the  connectives  in  response 
to  L7  activity  may  increase  the  fluid  resistance  of  their 
vascular  channels,  thereby  delaying  the  washout  of  the 
autoexcitatory  peptides  and  ensuring  that  mutual  exci- 
tation of  the  bag  cells  is  maximally  expressed  (Alevizos 
et  ai,  1991b).  (3)  The  increase  in  peristalsis  of  the  her- 
maphroditic duct  presumably  contributes  to  the  mixing 
of  the  eggs  with  the  secretory  products  of  the  duct,  as  well 
as  assisting  the  cilia  within  the  duct  in  moving  the  eggs 
to  the  caudal  end  of  the  genital  groove  (Alevizos  et  ai, 
1991c).  (4)  The  constriction  of  the  left  pedal/parapodial 
artery  shunts  arterial  blood  to  the  right  pedal/parapodial 
artery,  which  perfuses  the  genital  groove.  Such  an  effect 
could  help  support  the  metabolic  activity  of  the  cilia  lining 
the  groove,  which  move  the  eggs  several  cm  up  the  groove 
to  its  anterior  orifice,  from  which  they  are  deposited  on 
the  substrate.  (5)  In  addition  to  its  direct  synaptic  actions, 
R15  is  also  thought  to  have  a  neurosecretory  action  that 
influences  water  balance.  R 1 5  synthesizes  R 1 5«  1  peptide, 
a  38  amino  acid  neuropeptide  that  causes  an  increase  in 
net  water  retention  when  injected  into  the  animal  (Weiss 
et  ai.  1989).  R15  has  numerous  varicosities  that  appear 
to  release  into  systemic  vascular  spaces  (Rittenhouse  and 
Price,  1985),  leading  to  the  suggestion  that  R15  may  in- 
crease net  water  uptake  when  it  is  excited  by  the  bag  cells 
(Alevizos  et  ai,  1991c).  Such  an  effect  may  be  required 
to  counter  the  water  lost  in  egg  formation,  for  the  eggs 
are  fertilized  and  packaged  into  gelatinous  egg  capsules 
on  demand — i.e.,  in  response  to  the  bag  cell  burst 
(Thompson,  1976).  It  will  be  necessary  to  record  RIS's 
firing  pattern  during  spontaneous  egg  laying  in  the  intact 
animal  to  determine  the  actual  contributions  of  these  dif- 
ferent effects  of  R 1 5  activity  to  egg  laying  behavior. 

Each  of  the  four  direct  synaptic  actions  of  R15  can  be 
mimicked  by  R15«l  peptide,  and  the  peptide  probably 
mediates  them  when  R 1 5  bursts.  These  synaptic  actions 
are  unusual  in  that  they  decay  quite  rapidly  with  repeated 


272 


A.  ALEVIZOS  ET  AL. 


activation  of  R15.  An  example  of  this  synaptic  decrement 
is  shown  in  Figure  2,  for  the  R15-R25/L25  connections. 
Prolonging  the  R15  burst  period  to  greater  than  10  min 
has  no  added  effect  on  the  excitation  of  the  R25/L25  cells. 
The  fact  that  the  response  of  the  R25/L25  cells  to  direct 
application  of R15al  peptide  decreases  in  a  similar  fash- 
ion argues  against  depression  of  release  being  critical  for 
the  decrement  in  synaptic  transmission.  In  addition,  the 
R25/L25  cells  respond  normally  to  another  excitatory 
transmitter  when  the  response  to  R 1 5  is  depressed,  ruling 
out  non-specific  refractoriness  or  postsynaptic  inhibition 
as  contributing  to  this  synaptic  decrement.  Thus,  post- 
synaptic desensitization  appears  to  be  the  most  likely  ex- 
planation for  the  decay  of  R15's  direct  synaptic  actions 
on  the  R25/L25  network.  A  similar  conclusion  is  drawn 
from  the  actions  of  R15  on  L7,  on  the  hermaphroditic 
duct  and  on  the  arterial  muscle.  However,  in  the  case  of 
the  two  peripheral  tissues,  one  cannot  rule  out  muscular 
fatigue  as  a  contributor  to  response  decrement  (Alevizos 
el  al..  199 Ic). 

The  direct  synaptic  actions  of  R15  are  difficult  to  ob- 
serve in  vitro  without  taking  special  precautions.  They  are 
normally  chronically  depressed  by  the  profound  desen- 
sitization that  results  from  the  fact  that  R15  fires  spon- 
taneously at  a  high  rate  in  vitro.  Only  if  R15  is  silenced 
by  injecting  hyperpolarizing  current  for  1-2  h  does  the 
desensitization  decay,  unmasking  the  four  synaptic  actions 
described  above  (Alevizos  et  al..  1991a,b,c).  This  obser- 
vation has  two  important  implications.  First,  the  synaptic 
actions  of  other  spontaneously  active  neurons  may  be 
masked  if  they  undergo  profound  depression.  It  may  be 
necessary  to  silence  such  cells  for  a  long  time  to  restore 
their  synaptic  connections  to  a  level  where  they  can  be 
detected.  Second,  the  observation  that  R15's  synaptic  ac- 
tions rapidly  become  completely  depressed  results  in  a 
paradox.  How  can  these  actions  ever  be  expressed,  given 
that  R15  bursts  continuously  in  //;  vitro  experiments?  Are 
its  synaptic  connections  constantly  desensitized?  This 
question  was  addressed  in  chronic  recording  experiments, 
in  which  the  axon  of  R15  was  recorded  from  in  intact, 
freely  moving  animals  (Alevizos  et  al.,  199 la).  It  was 
found  that  R 1 5  does  not  burst  spontaneously  in  the  intact 
animal  (Fig.  3).  Given  that  R15  is  inactive  in  the  intact 
animal  and  is  excited  by  the  bag  cells  in  vitro,  it  has  been 
suggested  that  R 1 5  is  a  conditional  burster  that  is  switched 
to  the  bursting  mode  by  the  bag  cell  burst  that  triggers 
egg  laying  (Alevizos  et  al..  199 la).  Although  preliminary 
results  support  this  hypothesis,  it  has  not  yet  been  deter- 
mined whether  R 1 5  fires  during  a  spontaneous  egg  laying 
episode  in  the  intact  animal. 

The  mechanism  that  generates  spontaneous  bursts  in 
R15  has  been  studied  extensively  (Adams,  1985;  Adams 
andLevitan,  1985;  Lewis,  1988;  Thompson  et  al..  1986). 
In  its  broad  details  it  resembles  the  burst  generating 


-    5  MIN  N=10 
K30  MIN  N=6 


10  -| 


CO 


CO 
EC 


m 

LT> 


in 
c\i 

tr 


-10  MIN  N=12 
-60  MIN  N  =  10 


R15  FIRING 


10 


50 
TIME  (MIN) 


90 


Figure  2.  Modulation  of  the  frequency  of  respiratory-  pumping  by 
R15  decays  during  prolonged  R15  activity.  When  R15  was  allowed  to 
burst  spontaneously  for  various  amounts  of  time  after  a  2-h  period  of 
hyperpolanzation,  it  produced  a  long-lasting  increase  in  the  frequency 
of  respirator,  pumping.  The  amplitudes  of  the  maximum  effect  and  the 
time  courses  of  decay  of  these  increases  were  not  significantly  different 
for  the  10-.  30-,  and  60-min  firing  periods,  while  the  effect  produced  by 
the  5-min  firing  was  significantly  smaller  in  both  amplitude  and  duration. 
These  data  indicate  that  the  maximum  effect  of  R 1 5  bursting  is  exerted 
within  the  first  5-10  min  of  R15  bursting,  beyond  which  the  response 
is  independent  of  R 1 5  activity.  There  was  no  trend  for  the  firing  rate  of 
R 1 5  to  slow  down  over  the  course  of  the  long  burst  periods  (Alevizos  et 
al..  199  la). 


mechanism  described  above  for  the  LUQ  cells.  Kramer 
and  Levitan  (1990)  have  demonstrated  that  modulation 
of  R 1 5  bursting  by  egg  laying  hormone  is  most  effective 
when  R 1 5  is  inactive,  consistent  with  the  observation  that 
R15  is  silent  in  the  intact  animal. 

Conclusions 

It  is  interesting  to  see  whether  a  comparison  of  these 
five  types  of  bursting  neurons  leads  to  any  conclusions 
about  how  the  properties  of  each  class  relates  to  its  func- 
tional role.  Even  with  this  relatively  small  sample  of  cell 
types,  a  few  generalizations  do  emerge  from  such  a  com- 
parison. 

Is  there  a  difference  between  individual  bursters,  which 
are  not  coupled  to  other  bursting  cells  (L10.  the  LUQ 
cells,  and  R 1 5 )  and  population  bursters,  which  fire  as  part 
of  a  population  burst  (the  bag  cells  and  the  R25/L25  cells)? 
The  two  classes  are  alike  in  one  respect — both  individual 
bursters  and  population  bursters  can  have  endogenous 
pacemaker  mechanisms  (the  LUQ  cells,  L10,  R15  and 
the  R25/L25  cells).  They  differ,  however,  in  their  ability 
to  generate  episodic  bursts.  In  the  in  vitro  preparations 
that  have  been  examined  so  far.  individual  bursters  do 
not  seem  to  fire  in  isolated  bursts.  The  population  bursters 
(the  bag  cells  and  the  R25/L25  cells),  however,  by  virtue 
of  the  positive  feedback  chemical  and  electrical  connec- 


BURSTING  NEURONS  IN  APLYSIA 


273 


A 

NERVE 
BRANCH 


10uV 


6sec 


B 

NERVE 
BRANCH 


R15 


20mV 


6sec 


Figure  3.  R15  does  not  hurst  spontaneously  in  the  intact  animal.  An  extracellular  electrode  chronically 
implanted  in  the  subject  was  used  to  record  activity  from  a  small  branch  of  the  pericardial  nerve  that  contains 
R 1 5  processes.  (A)  In  the  intact  animal  there  was  no  bursting  activity  of  R 1 5  recorded  from  the  nerve 
branch.  Three  random  1.5-min  samples  are  shown  from  a  2-h  recording.  (B)  At  the  end  of  the  experiment 
the  animal  was  sacrificed,  and  the  abdominal  ganglion  was  dissected  from  the  animal,  along  with  the  electrode 
still  attached  to  the  nerve  branch.  Bursting  activity  appeared  in  the  nerve  of  the  isolated  ganglion.  Intracellular 
recording  from  the  R15  soma  confirmed  that  the  bursting  activity  in  the  nerve  was  due  to  R15  (n  =  9) 
(Alevizosrtu/..  199  la). 


tions  within  each  population,  are  well  adapted  to  generate 
single  bursts  in  response  to  brief  volleys  of  excitatory  syn- 
aptic  input. 

The  excitatory  and  inhibitory  chemical  synaptic  con- 
nections between  members  of  a  population  of  bursting 
cells  also  appear  to  extend  the  range  of  possible  burst  du- 
rations and  intensities.  While  typical  burst  durations  for 
the  individual  bursters  are  about  5-30  s  (at  15°C),  the 
bag  cell  bursts  last  1 5-30  min.  At  the  other  extreme,  al- 
though the  burst  duration  of  the  R25/L25  network  is  quite 
variable,  the  high  frequency  terminal  phase  of  the  burst, 
which  actually  drives  the  motoneurons,  lasts  only  1-2  s. 
The  excitatory  synaptic  connections  between  the  R25/ 
L25  cells  also  contribute  significantly  to  the  high  firing 
frequencies  that  these  cells  attain  during  a  burst — as  high 
as  25-40  Hz.  In  contrast,  individual  bursters  generally 
reach  peak  firing  frequencies  of  only  1-3  Hz  during  a 
burst.  The  role  of  chemical  connections  in  shaping  the 
burst  can  also  be  extended  to  controlling  the  refractory 
period  by  the  activation  of  second  messenger  systems  that 
generate  long-lasting  effects.  In  the  case  of  the  bag  cells. 


the  refractory  period  can  be  made  to  last  as  long  as  18- 
24  h  in  this  way. 

The  shaping  of  the  duration  of  the  population  bursts 
of  the  bag  cells  and  the  R25/L25  cells  seems  to  have  clear 
functional  consequences.  In  the  case  of  the  bag  cells,  the 
long-lasting  bursts  with  the  gradual  increase  in  spike  width 
appear  to  provide  a  large  safety  margin  for  release  of  an 
effective  dose  of  egg  laying  hormone  into  the  circulation. 
For  the  R25/L25  cells,  the  very  brief,  high  frequency  burst 
is  well  suited  for  driving  intense,  synchronous  contractions 
of  the  mantle  organs,  thereby  optimizing  the  pumping 
action. 

It  is  more  problematic  to  understand  the  significance 
of  the  bursting  patterns  of  the  individual  bursters.  The 
bursts  generated  by  one  of  them,  L10,  does  have  an  ob- 
vious function.  Each  burst  elicits  a  phasic  opening  of  the 
renal  pore.  But  whether  the  pore  actually  opens  this  way 
in  vivo  remains  to  be  determined.  In  addition,  it  is  not 
clear  why  the  other  two  classes  of  individual  bursters  fire 
in  a  bursting  mode.  The  synaptic  actions  generated  by 
R15  and  the  LUQ  cells  are  so  slow  that  their  follower 


274 


A.  ALEVIZOS  ET  AL. 


cells  effectively  integrate  their  firing  patterns.  That  is,  there 
is  no  reflection  of  the  phasic  bursting  patterns  of  R15  and 
the  LUQ  cells  on  any  of  the  nerve  or  muscle  cells  on 
which  they  synapse.  This  raises  the  question  of  why  R15 
and  the  LUQ  cells  burst,  rather  than  firing  in  steady  trains. 
Three  possible  explanations  come  to  mind:  ( 1 )  They  may 
have  other,  more  phasic  synaptic  actions.  For  example, 
R 1 5  has  transient  synaptic  actions  on  other  neurons  in 
the  abdominal  ganglion.  These  effects  are  not  observed 
in  all  preparations,  however,  suggesting  that  they  may  be 
gated  by  some  undetermined  physiological  variable  (Ale- 
vizos  and  Koester,  1986;  Brown  and  Mayeri;  1987).  (2) 
The  release  properties  of  these  cells  may  be  such  that  brief 
bursts  of  activity  are  the  most  efficient  for  optimizing  re- 
lease. (3)  Synaptic  release  in  Aplysia  is  strongly  influenced 
by  the  level  of  membrane  potential  immediately  preceding 
initiation  of  an  action  potential.  Hyperpolarization  de- 
presses release  and  depolarization  enhances  release  (Shi- 
mahara  and  Peretz,  1978;  Shapiro  el  ai,  1980).  Perhaps 
the  slow  depolarizing  waves  of  membrane  potential  that 
generate  the  bursts  in  these  cells  are  conducted  electro- 
tonically  to  the  terminals,  where  they  may  modulate  spike- 
evoked  release.  However,  it  seems  unlikely  that  such 
changes  in  resting  potential  would  be  conducted  to  ter- 
minals in  the  periphery.  Therefore,  a  cell  like  R 1 5,  which 
makes  both  central  and  peripheral  synapses,  may  have 
quite  different  release  properties  at  its  synapses  within  the 
ganglion  compared  to  those  in  the  periphery.  If  there  does 
exist  a  difference  between  central  and  peripheral  release 
sites,  it  may  be  amplified  by  modulatory  inputs  to  R15 
such  as  the  one  from  the  bag  cells.  When  they  fire  in  a 
population  burst,  the  bag  cells  increase  the  depth  of  the 
depolarizing  pacemaker  waves  recorded  from  the  soma 
of  R15  (Mayeri  et  ai.  1979).  Thus,  within  the  ganglion, 
the  bag  cells  may  influence  release  of  peptides  from  R15 
by  two  mechanisms:  an  increase  in  spike  frequency  and 
modulation  of  the  slower  membrane  potential  trajectory 
between  bursts.  The  terminals  in  the  periphery,  however, 
are  likely  to  experience  only  the  increase  in  spike  fre- 
quency. 

Acknowledgments 

This  work  was  supported  by  NIH  grant  NS 14385. 

Literature  Cited 

Adams,  W .  B.  1985.  Slow  depolarizing  and  hyperpolanzing  currents 
which  mediate  bursting  in  Aplysia  neurone  R15.  /  Physio!  360:  51- 
68. 

Adams,  W.  B.,  and  I.  B.  Levitan.  1985.  Voltage  and  ion  dependence 
of  the  slow  currents  which  mediate  bursting  in  Aplysia  neurone  Rl  5. 
J.  PhysioL  360:  69-93. 

Alevizos,  A.,  and  J.  Koester.  1986.  Identified  gill  motoneuron  L7  of 
Aplysia  is  a  multimodal  motoneuron  that  also  innervates  the  cardio- 
vascular system.  Soc.  Neurosci.  Abslr.  12:  499. 


Alevizos,  A.,  C.  H.  Bailey,  M.  Chen,  and  J.  Koester.  1989.  Innervation 
of  vascular  and  cardiac  muscle  of  Aplysia  by  multimodal  motoneuron 
L7.  J.  Neiirophysuil.  61:  1053-1063, 

Alevizos,  A.,  K.  R.  Weiss,  and  J.  KoesCer.  199la.  Synaptic  actions  of 
identified  peptidergic  neuron  R15  in  Aplysia:  I.  Activation  ot  respi- 
ratory pumping.  J.  Neurosci.  (in  press). 

Alevizos,  A.,  K.  R.  Weiss,  and  J.  Koesler.  1991b.  Synaptic  actions  of 
identified  peptidergic  neuron  R 1 5  in  Aplysia:  II.  Contraction  of  pleu- 
roabdominal  connectives  mediated  by  motoneuron  L7.  J.  Neurosci. 
(in  press). 

Alevizos,  A.,  K.  R.  Weiss,  and  J.  Koester.  199lc.  Synaptic  actions  of 
identified  peptidergic  neuron  RI5  in  Aplysia:  III.  Activation  of  the 
large  hermaphroditic  duct.  J.  Neurosci.  (in  press). 

Andrews,  E.  B.  1988.  Excretory  systems  of  molluscs.  Pp.  381-448  in 
The  Mollusca.  Vol.  1 1.  A.  S.  M.  Saleuddin  and  K.  M.  Wilbur,  eds. 
Academic  Press.  Inc.,  New  York. 

Branton,  D.  VV.,  E.  Mayeri,  P.  Browne!!,  and  S.  S.  Simon.  1978. 
Evidence  for  local  hormonal  communication  between  neurones  in 
Aplysia.  Nature  274:  70-72. 

Brown,  R.  O.,  and  E.  Mayeri.  1987.  Central  actions  of  R 15.  a  putative 
peptidergic  neuron  in  Aplysia.  J.  Neurobiol.  18:  3-13. 

Brown,  R.  O.,  and  E.  Mayeri.  1989.  Positive  feedback  by  autoexcitatory 
neuroendocrine  bag  cells  of  Aplysia.  J  \enrwci.  9:  1443-1451. 

Byrne,  J.  1983.  Identification  and  initial  characterization  of  a  cluster 
of  command  and  pattern-generating  neurons  underlying  respiratory 
pumping  in  Aplysia  californica.  J.  Nenropliysiol.  49:  491-508. 

Byrne,  J.,  and  J.  Koester.  1978.  Respiratory  pumping:  neuronal  control 
of  a  centrally  commanded  behavior  in  Aplysia.  Brain  Res.  143:  87- 
105. 

Cobbs,  J.  S..  and  H.  M.  Pinsker.  1982.  Role  of  bag  cells  in  egg  de- 
position of  Aplysia  hrasiliana.  I.  Comparison  of  normal  and  elicited 
behaviors.  J.  Comp.  Physiol.  147:  523-535. 

Conn,  P.  J.,  J.  S.  Strong,  E.  M.  Azhderian,  A.  C.  Nairn,  P.  Greengard, 
and  L.  K.  Kaczmarek.  1988.  Protein  kinase  inhibitors  selectively 
block  phorbol  ester  or  forskolin-induced  changes  in  excitability  of 
Aplysia  neurons.  /  Neurosci.  9:  480-487. 

Croll,  R.  P.  1985.  Sensory  control  of  respiratory  pumping  in  Aplysia 
californica.  J  E\p.  Biol.  117:  15-27. 

De  Riemer,  S.  A.,  J.  A.  Strong,  K.  A.  Albert,  P.  Greengard.  and  L.  K. 
Kaczmarek.  1985.  Enhancement  of  calcium  current  in  Aplysia 
neurons  by  phorbol  ester  and  protein  kinase  C.  Nature  313:  313- 
316. 

Dudek,  F.E..J.S.  Cobbs,  and  H.M.  Pinsker.  1979.  Bag  cell  electrical 
activity  underlying  spontaneous  egg  laying  in  freely  behaving  Aplysia 
brasiliana.  J.  Neurophysiol.  42:  804-817. 

Dudek,  F.  E.,  G.  Weir,  J.  Acosta-Urquidi,  and  S.  S.  Tobe.  1980.  A 
secretion  from  neuroendocrine  bag  cells  evokes  release  in  vitro  from 
ovotestis  of  Aplysia  californica.  Gen.  Comp.  Endocrinol.  40:  241- 
244. 

Eberly,  L.,  J.  Kanz,  C.  Taylor,  and  H.  Pinsker.  1981.  Environmental 
modulation  of  a  central  pattern  generator  in  freely  behaving  Aplysia. 
Behav  Neural  Biol.  32:  21-34. 

Eberly,  L.  B.,  and  H.  M.  Pinsker.  1984.  Neuroethological  studies  of 
reflex  plasticity  in  intact  Aplysia.  Behav.  Neurosci.  98:  609-630. 

Ferguson,  G.  P.,  A.  Ter  Maat,  D.  W.  Parsons,  and  H.  M.  Pinsker. 
1989.  Egg  laying  in  Aplysia:  I.  Behavioral  patterns  and  muscle  ac- 
tivity of  freely  behaving  animals  after  selectively  elicited  bag  cell  dis- 
charges. ./  Comp.  PhysioL  A  164:  835-847. 

Frazier,  W.  T.,  E.  R.  Kandel.  I.  Kupfermann.  R.  Waziri,  and  R.  E.  Cog- 
geshall.  1967.  Morphological  and  functional  properties  of  identified 
neurons  in  the  abdominal  ganglion  of  Aplysia  californica.  J.  Neu- 
rophysiol. 30:  1288-1351. 

Kaczmarek,  L.  K.,  and  J.  A.  Kauer.  1983.  Calcium  entry  causes  a  pro- 
longed refractory  period  in  peptidergic  neurons  of  Aplysia.  J.  Neurosci. 
3:  2230-2239. 


BURSTING  NEURONS  IN  APLYSIA 


275 


Kandel,  E.  R.  1976.     The  Cellular  Basis  of  Behavior   Freeman.  San 

Francisco. 
Kan/.  J.  E.,  and  W.  D.  Quasi.  1990.     Respiratory  pumping  seizure:  a 

newly  discovered  spontaneous  stereotyped  behavior  pattern  in  the 

opisthobranch  mollusc  Aplysia  californica.  J  Comp.  Physio!  A  166: 

619-627. 
Kauer,  J.  A.,  and  L.  K.  Kac/marek.  1985.     Peptidergic  neurons  of  Aplysia 

lose  their  response  to  cAMP  during  a  prolonged  refractory  period. 

J.  Neurosci.  5:  1339-1345. 
Kleinfeld.  D.,  T.  D.  Parsons,  F.  Raccuia-Behling,  B.  M.  Salzberg,  and 

A.  L.  Obaid.  1990.     Foreign  connections  are  formed  in  vitro  by 

Aplvsia  californica  interneurone  L10  and  its  in  vivo  followers  and 

non-followers.  J.  E.\p.  BioL  154:  237-255. 
Koester,  J.  1989.     Chemically  and  electrically  coupled  intcrneurons 

mediate  respiratory  pumping  in  Aplysia.  J.  Neurophysiol.  62:  I  1  1 3- 

1126. 
Koester,  J.,  and  A.  Alevizos.  1989.     Innervation  of  the  kidney  of  Aplysia 

by  L10,  the  LUQ  cells,  and  an  identified  peripheral  motoneuron.  J. 

Neurosci.  9:  4078-4088. 
Koesler,  J.,  and  E.  R.  Kandel.  1977.     Further  identification  of  neurons 

in  the  abdominal  ganglion  of  Aplysia  using  behavioral  criteria.  Brain 

Res  121:  1-20. 
Koester,  J.,  E.  Mayeri,  G.  Liebeswar,  and  E.  R.  Kandel.  1974.     Neural 

control  of  circulation  in  Aplysia.  II.  Interneurons.  /  Neurophysiol. 

37:  476-496. 
Kramer,  R.  H.,  and  R.  S.  Zucker.  1985a.     Calcium-dependent  inward 

current  in  Aplysia  bursting  pacemaker  neurones.  J   Physiol  362: 

107-130. 
Kramer,  R.  H.,  and  R.  S.  Zucker.  1985b.     Calcium-induced  inactivation 

of  calcium  current  causes  the  inter-burst  hyperpolanzation  of  Aply.siu 

bursting  neurones.  J.  Physiol  362:  131-160. 
Kramer,  R.  H.,  and  I.  B.  Levitan.  1990.     Activity-dependent  neuro- 

modulation  in  Aplysia  neuron  Rl  5:  intracellular  calcium  antagonizes 

neurotransmitter  responses  mediated  by  cAMP. ./.  Neurophysiol.  63: 

1075-1088. 

Kupfermann,  1.,  and  E.  R.  Kandel.  1969.     Neuronal  controls  of  a  be- 
havioral response  mediated  by  the  abdominal  ganglion  of  Aplysia 

Science  164:  847-850. 
Kupfermann,  I.,  and  E.  R.  Kandel.  1970.     Electrophysiological  properties 

and  functional  interconnections  of  two  symmetrical  neurosecretory 

clusters  (bag  cells)  in  abdominal  ganglion  of  Aplysia.  J.  Neurophysiol 

33:  865-876. 
Lewis,  D.  V.  1988.     Calcium-activated  inward  spike  after-currents  in 

bursting  neurone  R15  of  Aplysia.  J  Physiol.  396:  285-302. 
Mayeri,  E.,  P.  Brownell,  W.  D.  Branton,  and  S.  B.  Simon.  1979. 

Multiple,  prolonged  actions  of  neuroendocrine  bag  cells  on  neurons 

in  Aplysia.  I.  Effects  on  bursting  pacemaker  neurons.  J.  Neurophysiol. 

42:  1165-1184. 


Mayeri,  E.,  and  B.  S.  Rothman.  1985.  Neuropeptides  and  the  control 
of  egg-laying  behavior  in  Aplysia.  Pp.  285-301  in  Model  Neural  Net- 
works and  Behavior.  A.  I.  Selverston.  ed.  Plenum  Press.  New  York. 

Pinsker,  M.  H.,  and  F.  E.  Dudek.  1977.  Bag  cell  control  of  egg  laying 
in  freely  behaving  Aplysia.  Science  197:  490-493. 

Pinsker,  H.,  I.  Kupfermann,  V.  Castellucci,  and  E.  R.  Kandel. 
1970.  Habhuation  and  dishabituation  of  the  gill-withdrawal  reflex 
in  Aplysia.  Science  167:  1740-1742. 

Rittenhouse.  A.  R.,  and  C.  H.  Price.  1985.  Peripheral  axons  of  the 
parabolic  burster  neuron  R15.  Brain  Res.  333:  330-335. 

Rothman,  B.  S.,  E.  Mayeri,  R.  O.  Brown,  P.-M.  Yuan,  and  J.  E.  Shively. 
1983a.  Primary  structure  and  neuronal  effects  of  alpha-bag  cell 
peptide.  a  second  candidate  neurotransmitter  encoded  by  a  single 
gene  in  bag  cell  neurons  of  Aplysia.  Proc.  Nat.  Acad.  Sci.  USA  80: 
5753-5757. 

Rothman,  B.  S.,  G.  Weir,  and  F.  E.  Dudek.  1983b.  Egg  laying  hormone: 
direct  action  on  the  ovotestis  of  Aplysia.  Gen.  Comp.  Endocrinol.  52: 
131-141. 

Segal,  M.  M.,  and  J.  Koester.  1982.  Convergent  cholinergic  neurons 
produce  similar  postsynaptic  actions  in  Aplysia:  implications  for 
neural  organization.  J.  Neurophysiol.  47:  742-759. 

Shapiro,  E.,  V.  F.  Castellucci,  and  E.  R.  Kandel.  1980.  Presynaptic 
membrane  potential  affects  transmitter  release  in  an  identified  neuron 
in  Aplvsia  by  modulating  theCa**  and  K.*  currents.  Proc.  Nat.  Acad. 
Sci.  USA  77:  629-633. 

Shimahara.  T.,  and  B.  Peret/.  1978.  Some  potential  of  an  interneurone 
controls  transmitter  release  in  a  monosynoptic  pathway  in  Aplysia. 
Nature  273:  158-160. 

Strong,  J.  A.,  and  L.  K.  Kaczmarek.  1986.  Multiple  components  of 
delayed  potassium  current  in  peptidergic  neurons  of  Aplysia:  mod- 
ulation by  an  activator  of  adenylate  cyclase.  /.  Neurosci.  6:  8 14-822. 

Strong,  J.  A.,  A.  P.  Fox,  R.  W.  Tsien,  and  L.  K.  Kaczmarek. 
1987.  Stimulation  of  protein  kinase  C  recruits  covert  calcium 
channels  in  Aplysia  bag  cell  neurons.  Nature  325:  714-717. 

Thompson,  T.  E.  1976.  Biology  of  Opisthobranch  Molluscs.  Vol.  I.  The 
Ray  Society.  London. 

Thompson,  S.,  S.  J.  Smith,  and  J.  \V.  Johnson.  1986.  Slow  outward 
currents  in  molluscan  bursting  pacemaker  neurons:  two  components 
differing  in  temperature  selectivity.  J.  Neurosci.  6:  3169-3176. 

I'mitsu,  Y.,  H.  Matsumoto,  and  H.  Koike.  1987.  Active  contraction 
of  nerve  bundle  and  identification  of  a  nerve-contractor  motoneuron 
in  Aplysia.  J  Neurophysiol.  58:  1016-1034. 

Walters,  E.  T.,  and  M.  T.  Erickson.  1986.  Directional  control  and  the 
functional  organization  of  defensive  responses  in  Aplysia.  J  Comp. 
Physio/.  A  159:  339-351. 

Weiss,  K.  R.,  H.  Bayley,  P.  E.  Lloyd,  R.  Tenenbaum,  M.  A.  Gawinowicz 
Kolks,  L.  Buck,  E.  C.  Cropper,  S.  C.  Rosen,  and  I.  Kupfermann. 
1989.  Purification  and  sequencing  of  neuropeptides  contained  in 
neuron  R 1 5  of  Aplysia  californica.  Proc.  Nat.  Acad.  Sci.  USA  86: 
2913-2917. 


Reference:  Biol.  Bull.  180:  276-283.  (April,  1991) 


Contraction,  Serotonin-Elicited  Modulation,  and 

Membrane  Currents  of  Dissociated  Fibers 

of  Aplysia  Buccal  Muscle 

JEFFREY  L.  RAM,  FENG  ZHANG,  AND  LI-XIN  LIU 
Department  of  Physiology,  Wayne  State  University,  Detroit,  Michigan  48201 


Abstract.  Feeding  muscles  of  the  buccal  mass  of  Aplysia 
are  innervated  by  cholinergic  and  serotonergic  neurons. 
Buccal  muscle  15  contracts  in  response  to  acetylcholine 
(ACh).  During  feeding  arousal,  ACh-elicited  contraction 
of  muscle  15  is  potentiated  by  serotonin  (5-HT).  This  paper 
demonstrates  a  dissociated  cell  preparation  of  muscle  15 
in  which  cellular  mechanisms  regulating  contraction  can 
be  investigated. 

Dissociated  muscle  fibers  contracted  in  response  to  both 
KC1  and  ACh.  Serotonin  (10~6  M)  significantly  poten- 
tiated the  shortening  caused  by  both  KC1  and  ACh.  Po- 
tentiation  lasted  at  least  4  min,  similar  to  potentiation  in 
intact  muscles. 

Four  types  of  currents  recorded  by  patch  clamp  meth- 
ods are  illustrated.  With  540  m/U  KG  in  the  patch  elec- 
trode, stretch-activated  channels  having  a  chord  conduc- 
tance of  150  pS  are  observed  in  on-cell  patches.  In  whole 
cell  configuration,  ACh  elicits  inward  current  at  a  holding 
potential  of  -60  mV.  With  high  potassium  in  the  elec- 
trode, depolarization  elicits  an  outward  current.  The  volt- 
age-dependent outward  current  is  blocked  with  cesium  in 
the  electrode  and  4-aminopyridine  and  tetraethylam- 
monium  outside  the  cells.  The  remaining  voltage-depen- 
dent inward  current  is  calcium  dependent.  The  voltage- 
dependent  inward  and  outward  currents  are  activated 
within  the  range  of  depolarization  produced  by  ACh  and 
may  therefore  play  roles  in  regulating  contractile  responses 
elicited  by  ACh. 

Introduction 

Neurotransmitters  can  cause  both  direct  and  modula- 
tory  effects  on  target  tissues.  A  "modulatory  effect"  of  a 
neurotransmitter  is  one  in  which  the  transmitter  has  no 

Received  9  August  1990:  accepted  6  November  1990. 


immediate  effect,  but  rather,  it  modifies  the  influence  of 
other  effectors.  These  modulatory  influences  appear  to 
fine-tune  the  nervous  system  and  the  muscles  it  controls 
for  switching  between  different  behavioral  tasks.  The  same 
circuits  can  subserve  several  different  behaviors;  the  be- 
havioral output  depends  on  the  relative  weighting  of  dif- 
ferent synapses  and  the  excitability  of  circuit  elements. 
Modulation  of  muscle  is  part  of  this  integrated  scheme. 
Although  the  relative  strength  of  contractions  appropriate 
for  different  behaviors  could  be  manipulated  by  discrete 
relative  changes  in  motoneuron  activity,  an  additional, 
possibly  more  efficient  method  may  be  to  give  different 
"global  commands"  that  change  the  relative  strengths  of 
contractile  responses  in  ways  appropriate  for  particular 
behaviors.  An  example  of  a  modulatory  effect  of  a  neu- 
rotransmitter mediating  a  change  in  behavior  is  the  po- 
tentiating effect  of  serotonin  (5-HT)  on  buccal  mass  mus- 
cles of  Aplysia,  which  is  believed  to  mediate,  in  part,  feed- 
ing arousal. 

The  buccal  mass  muscles  of  Aplysia  are  smooth  muscles 
used  in  voluntary  feeding  movements.  These  muscles  are 
innervated  by  cholinergic,  peptidergic,  and  serotonergic 
neurons.  Acetylcholine  (ACh)  is  the  direct  effector  of  these 
muscles,  as  exemplified  by  the  contractile  response  to  ACh 
of  buccal  muscle  15.  Muscle  15,  also  known  as  the  acces- 
sory radular  closer  muscle  (Cohen  et  al.,  1978),  is  the 
most  intensively  studied  Aplysia  muscle.  15  is  innervated 
by  buccal  ganglion  neurons  B15  and  B16  (Cohen  et  al., 
1978;  Ram,  1983),  both  of  which  synthesize  ACh  (Cohen 
c/  al.,  1978)  as  well  as  several  peptides  (Cropper  et  at., 
1987,  1988).  In  addition,  15  is  innervated  by  the  seroto- 
nergic metacerebral  giant  cell  (MCG).  Activity  of  MCG 
causes  no  direct  response  of  the  muscle;  however,  it  po- 
tentiates subsequent  contractile  responses  to  B15  and  B16. 
This  modulation  is  achieved  largely  through  post-synaptic 
actions  on  the  muscle  (Weiss  el  a/.,  1978).  In  isolated  15 


276 


DISSOCIATED  MUSCLE  FIBERS  OF  APLYSIA 


111 


buccal  muscles  were  dissected  from  the  animal,  teased 
into  thin  strips,  and  incubated  for  2-4  h  at  28°C  in  Instant 
Ocean  containing  10  mAI  HEPES  (pH  7.0,  adjusted  with 
NaOH),  0. 1 5%  collagenase  (Sigma  Type  I).  0. 1%  soybean 
trypsin  inhibitor  (Sigma  Type  I-S),  and  1  //g/ml  leupeptin 


Figure  1.     Typical  dissociated  buccal  muscle  fiber.  At  the  right  is  a 
patch  electrode  pointing  to  the  fiber.  Scale:  calibration  marks  are  7.8 
apart. 


muscles  5-HT  produces  no  contractile  response  itself  but 
does  potentiate  ACh-elicited  contractions  (Ram  el  a/.. 
198 1 ).  MCGs  are  active  during  feeding  (Weiss  et  a/..  1978). 
Lesion  of  serotonergic  neurons  changes  (although  does 
not  completely  block)  feeding  motor  activity  (Rosen  et 
a/..  1983,  1989).  Thus,  the  modulatory  effect  of  the  se- 
rotonergic MCG  neurons  on  feeding  muscles  appears  to 
have  an  important  role  in  feeding  arousal. 

Although  previous  experiments  on  mechanisms  me- 
diating the  modulatory  effect  of  5-HT  on  buccal  muscles 
have  suggested  roles  for  cyclic  AMP  (Mandelbaum,  1980: 
Rametal..  1983,  1 984a)  and  calcium  ( Ram  etal..  1984b; 
Ram  and  Parti.  1985),  the  cellular  targets  of  these  me- 
diators have  not  been  determined.  For  example,  it  is  un- 
known whether  cyclic  AMP  or  calcium  modify  membrane 
mechanisms  such  as  ion  channels  or  change  the  sensitivity 
or  activity  of  contractile  proteins.  For  studying  effects  on 
contractile  proteins,  this  laboratory  developed  a  skinned 
muscle  preparation,  which  is  described  elsewhere  (Ram 
and  Patel,  1989).  To  study  membrane  mechanisms,  we 
developed  a  dissociated  muscle  fiber  preparation.  This 
paper  describes  the  contractile  properties  of  this  disso- 
ciated muscle  preparation,  including  its  modulatory  re- 
sponse to  5-HT,  and  demonstrates  its  suitability  for  patch 
clamp  analysis  of  single  channel  and  whole  cell  ionic  cur- 
rents. Preliminary  descriptions  of  some  of  these  data  have 
appeared  previously  (Ram  and  Liu,  1990;  Zhang  and 
Ram.  1990). 

Materials  and  Methods 

Individuals  ofAplysia  californica  (200-400  g)  were  ob- 
tained from  Marinus  (Long  Beach,  California)  and  main- 
tained at  18°C  in  Instant  Ocean  with  a  12:12  L:D  light 
cycle.  To  obtain  dissociated  muscle  fibers,  a  modification 
of  the  methods  of  Ishii  et  al.  (1986),  previously  used  to 
dissociate  muscle  fibers  in  Mytilus,  was  used.  Both  15 


Figure  2.  Serotonin  (5-HT)  potentiates  high  potassium-elicited  con- 
traction. Dissociated  muscle  fibers  in  a  small  chamber  (0.4  ml  volume) 
were  constantly  superfused  with  artificial  seawater  ( ASW)  at  5.5  ml/min. 
(A)  Fibers  at  rest.  (B)  Contractile  response  to  a  3-s  pulse  of  ASW  con- 
taining 100  mAI  KG.  All  but  the  fiber  in  the  upper  right  contracted. 
Fibers  returned  to  rest  length  at  the  end  of  the  pulse  of  KG.  (C)  Fibers 
at  the  end  of  1  mm  superfusion  with  ASW  containing  10~6  M  5-HT. 
Fibers  remained  at  rest  length.  (D)  Contractile  response  to  a  3-s  pulse 
of  100  mAI  KG  ASW.  identical  to  that  given  in  (B),  immediately  after 
1  min  superfusion  with  10~"  M  5-HT  ASW.  All  contracting  fibers  short- 
ened more  after  5-HT  treatment.  Scale:  calibration  marks  are  12  pm 
apart. 


278 


J.  L.  RAM  /:/  .11. 


150T 


100 


50 


KCI  contractions 

mean  resting  length  =  248  ±  28  fj.m.  n=9 

*  ,  p<0.05,  paired  t  compared  to  pro  — 5HT  (  —  2  min) 


-2024 
Time  post  — 5HT  (min) 

Figure  3.  Magnitude  and  time  course  of  5-HT  potentiation  of  K.C1- 
elicited  contractions.  Fibers  were  measured  in  images  similar  to  those 
illustrated  in  Figure  2.  High  potassium  (100  m.U  KCI  ASW)  pulses  were 
given  every  two  min.  Fibers  were  exposed  to  5-HT  (10~6  M  in  ASW) 
for  1  min  immediately  prior  to  the  0  time  point. 


(Sigma).  When  cells  began  appearing  in  the  medium,  fibers 
in  remaining  muscle  pieces  were  dispersed  by  gentle  trit- 
uration.  The  resultant  dissociated  cells  were  washed  by 
centrifuging  and  resuspending  them  in  wash  medium 
containing  all  ingredients  of  the  dissociating  medium  ex- 
cept collagenase.  Washed  cells  were  plated  onto  glass  cov- 
erslips  in  30-mm  plastic  petri  dishes  and  stored  at  4°C  in 
a  humidified  chamber.  Cells  were  usually  used  within  1- 
4  days,  although  viable,  contractile  cells  have  survived  for 
as  long  as  10  days  under  these  conditions.  Experiments 
were  done  at  room  temperature  (20-24°C)  after  allowing 
the  cells  to  warm  gradually  for  at  least  30  min. 

A  plexiglass  insert  having  a  central  hole  approximately 
1  cm  in  diameter  was  clamped  into  the  30-mm  petri  dish. 
The  insert  formed  a  small  chamber,  approximately  0.4 
ml  in  volume.  Medium  was  constantly  pumped  into  the 
chamber  (5.5  ml/min)  and  removed  by  suction  from  a 
surface  wick  opposite  the  inflow.  The  dish  was  mounted 
on  a  movable  stage  of  an  inverted  microscope.  The  shape 
and  movement  of  muscle  fibers  were  recorded  by  a  VHS 
camcorder  (RCA  CC310).  A  videotape  demonstrating 
many  of  the  contractile  and  electrophysiological  responses 
reported  in  this  paper  ("Dissociated  Muscle  Fibers  of 
Aplysia,"  by  J.  L.  Ram)  is  available  from  the  authors  upon 
request.  Morphometric  analysis  was  done  by  measuring 
still-images  on  the  tape  playback.  Photographs  were  made 
by  oscilloscope  camera  directly  off  the  TV  monitor. 

A  Dagan  8900  Patch  Clamp-Whole  Cell  Clamp  was 
used  for  single  channel  and  whole  cell  recording.  Data 
were  filtered  by  the  1  kHz  low-pass  filter  in  the  Dagan 
amplifier.  Electrodes  were  fabricated  from  Fisher  non- 
heparinized  hematocrit  glass,  polished  to  bubble  number 
3-4  (Corey  and  Stevens,  1985),  and  coated  with  Sylgard. 
Pipet  solutions  are  described  in  relevant  figure  captions. 


Electrical  stimuli  and  digital  recording  of  currents  were 
controlled  by  pCLAMP  software  (Axon  Instruments, 
Burlingame,  California). 

Results 

.Morphology  and  contraction 

The  typical  appearance  of  a  dissociated  fiber  from  buc- 
cal  muscle  15  is  illustrated  in  Figure  1 .  Dissociated  muscle 
fibers  were  spindle-shaped  and  ranged  from  5  to  25  nm 
in  diameter  and  up  to  a  mm  in  length.  The  widest  diameter 
usually  occurred  near  the  middle  of  the  fiber,  adjacent  to 
the  nucleus,  and  averaged  13.6  ±  0.9  ^m  (mean  ±  S.E., 
n  =  19).  The  average  diameter  of  the  fibers,  measured 
every  20  ^m  along  the  length  of  the  fiber,  was  10.8  ±  0.7 
urn.  The  average  length  of  fibers  at  rest  was  270  ±  10  ^m 
(n  =  37). 

Dissociated  muscle  fibers  contracted  in  response  to  KG. 
In  response  to  a  2-  or  3-s  pulse  of  ASW  containing  100 
mM  KCI,  22  fibers  that  had  average  resting  lengths  of  262 
±  13  iim  shortened  to  218  ±  12  ^m.  An  illustration  of  a 
subset  of  these  fibers  is  shown  in  Figure  2.  in  which  Figure 
2 A  shows  the  fibers  at  rest  and  Figure  2B  shows  the  max- 


B 


Figure  4.  Serotonin  (5-HT)  potentiates  ACh-elicited  contractions. 
The  procedure  is  identical  to  that  of  Figure  2  except  that  contraction 
was  elicited  by  a  3-s  pulse  of  10"4  M  ACh.  (A)  Fibers  at  rest.  (B)  Contractile 
response  to  ACh.  (C)  Contractile  response  to  identical  pulse  of  ACh  as 
in  (B)  immediately  after  1  min  superfusion  with  10~6  A/  5-HT  ASW. 
Scale:  calibration  marks  are  20  ^m  apart. 


DISSOCIATED  MUSCLE  FIBERS  OF  APLYS1A 


279 


ACh  contractions 

mean  resting  length  =  264  ±  20  ^zm,  n  =  6 

*  ,  p<0.05.  paired  t  compared  to  pre-5HT  (-2  mln) 


1SU- 

If 

* 
1 

|  "S  too 

1 

C      -t- 

to      o 

r    ° 

o    £ 

-C       C 

VI         O 

^      o 

h 

I 

O       i 

°     '       50 

c     01 

3       C 

o    ^i 

E    S 

n  . 

—  J— 

—  • 

— 

-  H  — 

1 

-2024 
Time  post-5HT  (min) 

Figure  5.  Magnitude  and  time  course  of  5-HT  potentiation  of  ACh- 
elicited  contractions.  Fibers  were  measured  on  the  field  in  Figure  4  and 
subsequent  images.  ACh  pulses  were  given  even1  -  rnin.  Fibers  were 
exposed  to  5-HT  ( ICT6  M  in  ASW)  for  1  min  immediately  prior  to  the 
0  time  point. 


imal  contraction  produced  by  a  3-s  pulse  of  KC1.  Follow- 
ing the  KC1  pulse,  fibers  relaxed  to  their  resting  lengths 
within  a  few  seconds. 

Serotonin  potentiated  the  contractile  response  to  KC1 
(Fig.  2D).  Figure  3  summarizes  data  from  nine  fibers  that 
were  exposed  to  1(T6  M  5-HT  for  1  min.  The  amount  of 
shortening  produced  by  KC1  pulses  was  almost  doubled 
following  5-HT,  and  the  effect  lasted  at  least  4  min,  similar 
to  the  long-lasting  potentiation  in  intact  muscles  produced 
by  5-HT  (Ram  et  ai,  1981).  The  fibers  remained  relaxed 
during  the  5-HT  application  (Fig.  2C). 

Similarly,  ACh  caused  contraction  of  dissociated  fibers, 
which  could  be  potentiated  by  5-HT.  Figure  4  shows  ACh- 
elicited  contractions  prior  to  5-HT  and  immediately  fol- 
lowing one  min  1(T6  M  5-HT.  Data  from  six  fibers,  sum- 
marized in  Figure  5,  show  the  significant  increase  in 
shortening  caused  by  5-HT  and  the  similar  time  course 
of  recovery  from  the  effects  of  5-HT  to  KCl-elicited  con- 
tractions. 


— vr-n — icr-n-TwryV|  -i'|-|    inrrv 


suction 


|10  pA 


1  sec 


Figure  6.  Channel  activity  in  on-cell  patches.  The  electrode  contained 
(in  roA/ )  406  KC1,  20  NaCl,  2  MgCl, ,  1 0  ATP,  0. 1  GTP.  1 0  glutathione, 
and  100  HEPES.  pH  7.0  with  KOFI.  Electrode  potential  was  identical 
to  the  bath  potential,  and  the  cell  was  at  resting  potential  (not  measured 
for  this  cell).  Suction  increased  the  opening  of  at  least  one  population 
of  large  channels,  conducting  approximately  10  pA  per  unitary  channel 
opening. 


> 

E 


150 


|  — 

120 

00 

LJ 

90 

2 

60 

0 

30 

g 
< 

N 


O 

a. 

LJ 
Q 


0 


25  ms 


Figure  7.  Current  through  stretch-activated  channels  in  an  on-cell 
patch,  measured  over  a  range  of  membrane  potentials.  The  electrode 
contained  540  m.U  K.C1.  Stretch  sensitivity  of  these  channels  was  dem- 
onstrated during  another  part  of  the  experiment  (not  illustrated  here). 
Membrane  potential  was  varied  by  changing  the  potential  of  the  patch 
electrode,  and  the  membrane  potential  is  given  as  the  change  from  resting 
potential.  Pipet  potential  was  held  at  each  potential  for  at  least  20  s. 
Channel  current  reversed  at  approximately  87  mV;  chord  conductance 
was  approximately  150  pS;  and  channel  opening  probability  was  inde- 
pendent of  membrane  potential. 


Single  channel  and  whole  cell  patch  clamp  recording 

Dissociated  buccal  muscle  fibers  were  suitable  for 
forming  gigaseals  for  single  channel  and  whole  cell  re- 
cording. With  high  potassium  in  the  patch  electrode,  on- 
cell  patches  revealed  the  presence  of  a  variety  of  channels 
conducting  inward  current  at  resting  potential,  including 
at  least  one  prominent  channel  that  could  be  activated 
by  increased  suction  on  the  electrode  (Fig.  6).  The  fre- 
quency of  the  opening  of  suction-activated  channels  in- 
creased with  negative  pressures  of  50-100  cm  HiO  (ap- 
proximately 40-80  mm  Hg),  as  measured  by  a  water  ma- 
nometer (n  =  3;  see  also  Ram  et  ai,  1990).  The  current 
through  unitary  channel  openings  of  the  most  prominent 
channel  activated  by  suction,  with  the  fiber  at  resting  po- 
tential and  the  electrode  at  bath  potential,  averaged  12 
±  1  pA  (n  =  7  patches).  The  chord  conductance  of  this 


280 


J.  L.  RAM  ET  AL 


10~5  M  ACh 


0.5  nA 


-Vm=-60  mV 
Vm=-80  mV 


10      15      20      25      30 
TIME  (s) 


35 


Figure  8.  Response  to  ACh  in  whole-cell  recording  configuration. 
Pipet  solution  contained  (in  mA/)  20  NaCl.  406  K.C1,  2  MgCU,  100 
HEPES.  5  EGTA,  5  MgATP,  0.1  NaGTP.  10  glutathione,  pH  7.0  (ad- 
justed with  KOH).  Fibers  were  constantly  superfused  with  artificial  sea- 
water.  Holding  potential  was  -80  mV  and  was  moved  to  -65  mV  every 
3s.  A  1 5-s  pulse  of  10~5  M  ACh  elicited  inward  current  at  both  membrane 
potentials. 


suction-activated  channel,  determined  by  eliciting  channel 
activity  at  several  different  holding  potentials,  averaged 
140  ±  20  pS  (n  =  3),  as  exemplified  by  the  patch  illustrated 
in  Figure  7. 

After  gigaseal  formation,  whole  cell  configuration  could 
be  achieved  by  applying  greater  suction  than  is  necessary 
to  activate  suction-activated  channels.  With  a  pipet  so- 
lution containing  high  potassium  and  other  ingredients 
meant  to  mimic  the  normal  intracellular  milieu  of  the 
fibers  (complete  composition  is  given  in  the  caption  to 
Fig.  8),  ACh  elicited  an  inward  current  (Fig.  8).  The  peak 
current  elicited  by  10  5  M  ACh  at  a  holding  potential  of 
-80  mV  was  -2.4  ±  0.5  nA  (n  =  20);  at  a  holding  po- 
tential of  -60  mV.  the  peak  current  averaged  - 1 .4  ±  0.4 
nA  (n  =  20).  Under  the  same  ionic  conditions,  depolar- 
ization activated  outward  current  (Fig.  9).  In  observations 
of  more  than  20  cells,  a  net  voltage  dependent  inward 
current  was  never  seen  under  conditions  of  having  high 
potassium  in  the  pipet  and  normal  sea  water  outside.  Oc- 
casionally, there  was  a  slight  delay  in  activation  of  outward 
current  (not  seen  in  Fig.  9),  possibly  indicating  an  initial 
counterbalancing  inward  current. 

Voltage-dependent  inward  current  can,  however,  be 
seen  under  conditions  that  block  potassium  channels. 
With  cesium  in  the  electrode  and  4-aminopyridine  and 
tetraethylammonium  in  the  extracellular  solution,  de- 
polarization elicited  an  inward  current.  The  peak  inward 
current  averaged  2.4  ±  .4  nA  (n  =  11  fibers).  As  illustrated 
in  Figure  10,  the  voltage  dependent  inward  current  was 
dependent  upon  calcium  in  the  extracellular  medium. 

Discussion 

This  paper  demonstrates  that  smooth  muscle  fibers  dis- 
sociated from  buccal  muscles  of  Aplysia  are  a  suitable 


preparation  for  studying  mechanisms  regulating  contrac- 
tion and  its  modulation.  First,  isolated  fibers  have  appro- 
priate contractile  responses:  They  contract  in  response  to 
both  high  potassium  and  ACh.  and  the  contractions  to 
both  are  potentiated  by  5-HT.  Second,  the  dissociated 
fibers  are  suitable  for  patch  clamp  analysis  of  single  chan- 
nel and  whole  cell  currents. 

Previous  studies  have  used  indirect  methods  for  inves- 
tigating the  roles  of  specific  ion  channels  in  regulating 
contraction  of  molluscan  muscles.  One  set  of  questions 
that  arise  concerns  the  sources  of  activator  calcium  in  the 
physiological  responses  to  neurotransmitters.  Is  contrac- 
tion dependent  upon  the  influx  of  extracellular  calcium? 
If  so,  are  the  channels  receptor  operated  or  voltage  de- 
pendent, and  are  they  specific  for  calcium?  Many  mol- 
luscan muscles  are  highly  dependent  upon  extracellular 
calcium  to  trigger  contraction.  For  example,  ACh-elicited 
contractions  of  buccal  muscle  El  (another  muscle  of  the 
Aplvsui  buccal  musculature  whose  contraction  is  poten- 
tiated by  5-HT)  fail  within  two  minutes  of  removal  of 
extracellular  calcium  (Ram  et  ai,  1984b).  Similarly,  cal- 
cium-dependence of  ACh-elicited  contractions  have  also 
been  demonstrated  in  a  non-spiking  muscle  of  Aplysia 
gill  (Reilly  and  Peretz,  1987)  and  in  four  different  pro- 


ms 


15  nA 


Membrane  Potential,  mV 
-  100  -50 


5 
0 


50 

i 


L  -5  nA 
Current 

Figure  9.  Voltage-dependent  outward  current  in  whole-cell  config- 
uration. Pipet  solution  and  external  medium  were  the  same  as  in  Figure 
8.  Holding  potential  was  -80  mV.  Currents  were  elicited  by  60-ms  pulses 
to  various  potentials,  from  -100  mV  to  +20  mV.  given  at  intervals  of 
1 .2  s.  Linear  leak  and  capacitative  transients  have  been  subtracted.  (Up- 
per) Typical  response  to  —30  mV.  (Lower)  Current  voltage  relationship 
for  peak  current  during  the  pulse. 


DISSOCIATED  MUSCLE  FIBERS  OF  APLYSIA 


281 


VH=-80  mV 


1   nA 


10  ms 


normal 
calcium 


Membrane  potential  (mV) 


-T     1   nA 


Figure  10.  Voltage-dependent  calcium  current  in  whole-cell  config- 
uration. Pipet  solution  contained  (in  mA/)  20  NaCl,  406  CsCI.  2  MgCl:. 
100  HEPES,  5  EGTA,  5  MgATP.  0.1  NaGTP.  10  glutathione.  pH  7.0 
(adjusted  with  CsOH).  External  medium  was  artificial  seawater  containing 
(in  m.U)  50  tetraethylammonium,  5  4-ammopyndine.  485  NaCl.  25 
MgSO4,  25  MgCl:,  10  KC1,  10  CaCI,,  and  10  HEPES,  pH  7.8  (adjusted 
with  NaOH).  For  0  calcium  medium,  the  CaCl:  was  left  out  and  all  other 
ingredients  increased  in  concentration  by  1%.  Holding  potential  was 
-80  mV.  Currents  were  elicited  by  60  ms  pulses  to  various  potentials, 
from  - 100  mV  to  + 10  mV  at  intervals  of  1.2  s.  Linear  leak  and  capac- 
itative  transients  have  been  subtracted.  (Upper)  Typical  responses  to 
depolarization  to  -30  mV  in  the  presence  and  absence  of  calcium. 
(Lower)  Current-voltage  relationships  for  peak  current  in  the  presence 
and  absence  of  calcium. 


boscis  muscles  of  the  marine  snail  Busycon  (Huddart  and 
Hill,  1988;  Huddart  el  ai.  1990a,  b;  Hill  and  McDonald- 
Ordzie.  1979;  Hill  el  ai,  1970).  Another  molluscan  mus- 
cle, the  anterior  byssus  retractor  muscle  (ABRM)  seems 
less  dependent  upon  extracellular  calcium  because  ACh- 
elicited  contractions  of  ABRM  are  not  abolished  by  the 
removal  of  extracellular  calcium  for  up  to  10  min  (Sugi 
and  Yamaguchi,  1976).  Recent  investigations  of  intra- 
cellular  calcium  levels  in  ABRM  using  the  calcium-sen- 
sitive fluorescent  indicator  FURA-2  have  shown  that  al- 
though extracellular  calcium  may  account  for  the  majority 
of  the  rise  in  intracellular  calcium  in  response  to  cholin- 


ergic  activation,  about  30%  of  the  increase  in  intracellular 
free  calcium  may  be  attributed  to  the  release  of  stored 
calcium  (Ishii  tf  ai.  1988). 

Studies  on  the  influx  of  calcium-45  in  response  to  ACh 
have  been  used  to  determine  whether  calcium  dependence 
of  contraction  involves  physical  movement  of  calcium 
into  the  cell.  ACh  stimulates  influx  of  calcium-45  into 
Aplysia  buccal  muscles  El  and  15  (Ram  and  Parti,  1985; 
Gole  el  ai.  1987);  however,  ACh  does  not  significantly 
increase  calcium-45  influx  into  ABRM  (Tameyasu  and 
Sugi.  1976).  The  lack  of  significant  ACh-stimulated  cal- 
cium-45 influx  in  ABRM  not  only  contrasts  with  the  ob- 
servations in  Aplysia  muscles  but  also  stands  in  apparent 
contradiction  with  FURA-2  measurements  showing  a 
significant  extracellular  dependence  of  the  ACh-stimu- 
lated rise  in  intracellular  calcium  in  ABRM  (see  above). 
Calcium-45  influx  measurements  are  inherently  more 
variable  than  FURA-2  measurements.  Therefore,  the  lack 
of  significant  effect  of  ACh  on  calcium-45  influx  in  ABRM 
probably  reflects  a  relatively  lower  importance  of  calcium 
influx  in  ABRM  compared  to  Aplysia  buccal  muscles 
rather  than  a  complete  absence  of  ACh-stimulated  influx. 

A  possible  route  for  calcium  entry  into  muscle  cells 
during  the  response  to  ACh  is  via  voltage-dependent  cal- 
cium channels.  As  discussed  below,  ACh  causes  depolar- 
ization of  molluscan  muscles.  Previous  evidence  that  de- 
polarization could  activate  voltage  dependent  calcium 
channels  included  demonstrating  that  another  depolar- 
izing stimulus,  a  high  potassium  medium,  causes  con- 
traction. High  potassium  induces  contractions  of  Busycon 
proboscis  retractor  muscles  (e.g..  Huddart  el  ai.  1990a. 
b),  ABRM  (e.g.,  TwarogandMuneoka,  1972),  and. Aplysia 
buccal  muscles  (Ram,  unpub.  data).  Furthermore,  high 
potassium  elicits  contraction  of  isolated  fibers,  as  described 
in  Aplysia  buccal  muscle  (Figs.  2  and  3,  this  paper)  and 
in  ABRM  (Ishii  el  ai.  1986),  unambiguously  proving  that 
contraction  elicited  by  high  potassium  is  a  direct  effect 
on  single  fibers  and  is  not  dependent  upon  either  release 
of  neurotransmitters  from  nerve  endings  in  the  muscle 
or  mechanical  or  electrical  coupling  between  fibers.  Ishii 
el  ai  (1988)  has  also  used  FURA-2  to  show  that  high 
potassium  causes  an  increase  in  intracellular  calcium  that 
is  completely  dependent  upon  extracellular  calcium. 

Blockers  of  voltage-dependent  calcium  channels  reduce 
the  contractile  responses  produced  by  both  ACh  and  high 
potassium.  Thus,  Huddart  el  ai  ( 1990b)  found  that  dilti- 
azam,  verapamil,  and  nifedipine  all  decrease  ACh-elicited 
contractions  of  Busycon  proboscis  muscles.  Similarly,  ni- 
fedipine reduces  ACh-elicited  contraction  of  Aplysia  buc- 
cal muscle  (Ram  and  Liu,  1990). 

The  above  indirect  evidence  for  the  existence  of  voltage- 
dependent  calcium  channels  is  now  supported  in  the 
present  paper  by  voltage  clamp  recordings  of  a  voltage- 
dependent  inward  current  that  is  dependent  on  extracel- 
lular calcium  (Fig.  10).  As  described  in  a  preliminary  re- 


282 


J.  L.  RAM  ET  AL. 


port  (Ram  and  Liu,  1990).  we  have  also  demonstrated 
that  this  current  is  partially  inhibited  by  nifedipine  and 
completely  blocked  by  lanthanum. 

For  voltage-dependent  calcium  channels  to  play  a  role 
in  mediating  ACh  responses,  it  must  also  be  shown  that 
the  range  of  membrane  potentials  at  which  a  voltage-de- 
pendent calcium  current  can  be  activated  is  within  the 
range  of  membrane  potentials  caused  by  ACh.  As  illus- 
trated in  Figure  10,  voltage-dependent  calcium  current 
begins  to  activate  with  depolarizations  to  —40  mV.  In 
other  cells  (data  not  shown),  voltage-dependent  calcium 
current  has  been  activated  with  depolarization  to  as  little 
as  -50  mV.  As  discussed  below,  ACh  can  depolarize  cells 
to  approximately  -35  mV.  Thus,  the  membrane  potential 
required  to  activate  voltage-dependent  calcium  channels 
is  clearly  within  the  range  of  depolarization  produced  by 
ACh  in  Aplysia  buccal  muscles.  This  paper  provides  the 
strongest  evidence  yet  that  activator  calcium  enters  mol- 
luscan  muscle  fibers  during  cholinergic  stimulation  by 
voltage-dependent  calcium  channels. 

This  paper  also  initiates  the  analysis  of  receptor-oper- 
ated channels  activated  by  ACh.  ACh  causes  depolariza- 
tion of  molluscan  muscle  fibers  in  clam  heart  (Wilkens 
and  Greenberg,  1973),  ABRM  (Twarog,  1954),  Bmycon 
proboscis  muscles  (Huddart  ct  ai,  1990b;  Hill  and  Licis, 
1985;  Hill  and  McDonald-Ordzie,  1979),  Aplysia  gill 
muscle  (Reilly  and  Peretz,  1987)  and  Aplysia  buccal  mus- 
cles (Ram  ct  ai.  1990).  Detailed  quantitative  studies  of 
Aplysia  buccal  muscle  revealed  (a)  an  average  resting  po- 
tential of -65  mV  (Gole  et  ai.  1987),  (b)  no  contraction 
elicited  with  depolarizations  less  than  approximately  10 
mV  above  rest,  (c)  a  non-linear  relationship  between  con- 
traction and  depolarization  in  which  increasing  ACh  be- 
yond a  certain  concentration  was  accompanied  by  in- 
creasing contraction  with  little  or  no  further  increase  in 
depolarization,  and  (d)  maximal  ACh-elicited  depolariza- 
tion of  approximately  30  mV  above  rest  (Ram  et  ai, 
1990). 

The  limit  on  depolarization  produced  by  ACh  to  only 
30  mV  above  rest  may  result  from  several  mechanisms. 
One  possibility  is  that  the  reversal  potential  for  ACh-ac- 
tivated  channels  is  only  30  mV  above  rest.  An  alternative 
explanation  is  that  depolarization  activates  voltage-de- 
pendent potassium  channels  that  act  as  an  effective  brake 
on  further  depolarization  even  in  the  face  of  activation 
of  more  ACh-activated  channels.  Data  in  this  paper  show 
that  analysis  of  this  question  is  feasible.  As  expected  for 
a  depolarizing  stimulus,  ACh  activates  inward  current 
(Fig.  8).  The  reversal  potential  of  the  ACh  response  is 
under  investigation  (Ram  and  Liu,  1990).  Furthermore, 
Figure  9  demonstrates  that  the  voltage-dependent  outward 
current  is  activated  within  the  voltage  range  elicited  by 
ACh.  The  voltage-dependent  outward  current  is  un- 
doubtedly potassium  because  it  is  blocked  by  TEA  and 
4-AP  outside  the  cell  and  Cs  in  the  electrode  (Fig.  10). 


This  paper  also  demonstrates  that  buccal  muscle  fibers 
contain  stretch-activated  channels.  Because  these  channels 
were  observed  with  on-cell  patch  electrodes  containing 
only  KC1,  the  inward  currents  illustrated  are  almost  cer- 
tainly due  to  potassium  current.  Similarly,  stretch-acti- 
vated channels  conducting  primarily  potassium  ions  have 
been  reported  previously  in  molluscan  neurons  (Morris 
and  Sigurdson,  1989;  Sigurdson  and  Morris,  1989)  and 
cardiac  muscle  (Brezden  and  Gardner,  1986;  Sigurdson 
ct  ai.  1987).  Stretch-activated  channels  that  are  somewhat 
less  selective  for  potassium  have  been  reported  in  various 
mammalian  tissues,  including  skeletal  muscle  (Guharay 
and  Sachs,  1984)  and  smooth  muscle  (Kirberrt  ai.  1988). 

In  future  experiments  it  should  be  possible  to  determine 
whether  5-HT  potentiates  contraction  of  dissociated 
muscle  fibers  by  modifying  the  ionic  currents  illustrated 
here.  One  indication  in  a  molluscan  muscle  that  5-HT 
may  change  membrane  currents  of  molluscan  muscles  is 
that,  in  ABRM.  5-HT  potentiates  the  rise  in  intracellular 
calcium  caused  by  100  mA/KCl  (Ishii  et  ai.  1989).  The 
rise  in  intracellular  calcium  in  response  to  KC1  is  com- 
pletely dependent  upon  extracellular  calcium  (Ishii  et  ai, 
1988)  and  is  presumed  to  be  due  to  voltage-dependent 
calcium  current,  similar  to  the  current  described  in  this 
paper  in  Figure  10.  In  addition,  5-HT  might  also  be  mod- 
ifying potassium  channels,  receptor-operated  channels, 
and  stretch-activated  channels. 

Acknowledgments 

This  work  was  supported  by  the  Muscular  Dystrophy 
Association  and  NIH  grant  RR-08167.  I  am  indebted  to 
R.  B.  Hill  for  his  critical  comments  on  the  manuscript. 

Literature  Cited 

Brezden,  B.  L.,  and  D.  R.  Gardner.  1986.  A  potassium  selective  channel 
in  isolated  Lymnaca  stagnalis  heart  muscle  cells.  J.  Exp.  Bio/.  123: 
175-189. 

Cohen,  J.  L.,  K.  R.  Weiss,  and  I.  Kupfermann.  1978.  Motor  control 
of  buccal  muscles  in  Aplysia.  J.  Neitmphys.  41:  157-180. 

Corey,  D.,  and  C.  S.  Stevens.  1985.  Science  and  technology  of  patch- 
recording  electrodes.  Pp.  53-68  'm  Single  Channel  Recording,  B.  Sak- 
mann  and  E.  Neher,  eds.  Plenum  Press,  New  York. 

Cropper,  E.  C,  M.  \V.  Miller,  R.  Tenenbaum,  M.  A.  G.  Kolks,  I.  Kup- 
fermann, and  K.  R.  Weiss.  1988.  Structure  and  action  of  buccalin: 
a  modulator,  neuropeptide  localized  to  an  identified  small  cardioac- 
tive  peptide-containing  cholinergic  motor  neuron  of  Aplysia  califor- 
nicii.  Proc.  Nal.  Acad.  Sci.  85:  6177-6181. 

Cropper,  E.  C.,  R.  Tenenbaum,  M.  A.  G.  Kolks,  I.  Kupfermann,  K.  R. 
Weiss.  1987.  Myomodulin:  a  bioactive  neuropeptide  present  in  an 
identified  cholinergic  buccal  motor  neuron  of  Aplysia.  Proc.  Nal. 
Acad.  Sci.  84:  5483-5486. 

Gole,  D..  I..  Munday,  M.  Kreiman,  and  J.  L.  Ram.  1987.  Caffeine 
effects  on  buccal  muscles  of  Aplysia.  Camp.  Biochciu.  Physial.  88C: 
313-318. 

Guharay,  F.,  and  K.  Sachs.  1984.  Stretch-activated  single  ion  channel 
currents  in  tissue-cultured  embryonic  chick  skeletal  muscle.  /  Physiol. 
352:685-701. 


DISSOCIATED  MUSCLE  FIBERS  OF  APLYSIA 


283 


Hill,  R.  B.,  M.  J.  Greenberg,  H.  Irisawa,  and  II.  Nomura. 
1970.  Electromechanical  coupling  in  a  molluscan  muscle,  the  radula 
protractor  of  Busyci >n  canaliciilatum  J  Exp  Zoo/  174:331-348. 

Hill,  R.  B.,  and  P.  Licis.  1985.  Lanthanum  and  caffeine  affect  both 
membrane  responses  and  force  in  a  molluscan  muscle,  the  radular 
protractor  of  Busycon  cana/iculatum.  Comp.  Biochem.  Physio/  82C: 
363-376. 

Hill,  R.  B.,  and  P.  K.  McDonald-Ordzie.  1979.  Ionic  dependence  of 
the  response  to  acetylcholine  of  a  molluscan  buccal  muscle:  the  radula 
protractor  of  Busycon  cana/iculatum  Comp  Biochcm  Physiol  62C: 
19-30. 

Huddart,  H.,  D.  D.  Brooks,  R.  B.  Hill,  and  R.  I.ennard.  1990a.  Diversity 
of  mechanical  responses  and  their  possible  underlying  mechanisms 
in  the  proboscis  muscles  of  Busycon  canaliciilatum.  J  Comp.  Physiol. 
B  159:  717-725. 

Huddart,  H.,  D.  D.  Brooks,  R.  Lennard,  and  R.  B.  Hill.  I990b.  Unusual 
responses  of  proboscis  muscles  of  Busycon  canaliculatum  to  some 
calcium  antagonist  agents.  J  Comp.  Physiol.  B  159:  727-738. 

Huddart,  H.,  and  R.  B.  Hill.  1988.  Electrochemical  uncoupling  in  a 
molluscan  muscle  examined  b>  the  sucrose  gap  technique.  The  effect 
of  calcium  antagonist  and  agonist  agents.  J  Comp.  Physiol.  B  158: 
501-512. 

Ishii,  N.,  T.  Takakuua,  and  K.  Takahashi.  1986.  Isolation  of  acetyl- 
choline-sensitive  smooth  muscle  cells  from  a  molluscan  catch  muscle. 
Comp.  Biochem.  Physiol.  84C:  1-6. 

Ishii,  N.,  A.  W.  M.  Simpson,  and  C.  C.  Ashley.  1988.  Carbachol  and 
KCl-induced  changes  in  intracellular  free  calcium  concentration  in 
isolated,  fura-2  loaded  smooth-muscle  cells  from  the  anterior  byssus 
retractor  muscle  of  Mylilus  edu/is  Biochcm.  Bmphys.  Re*.  Cumin. 
153:  683-689. 

Ishii,  N.,  A.  W.  M.  Simpson,  and  C.  C.  Ashley.  1989.  Effects  of  5- 
hydroxytryptamine  (serotonin)  and  forskolin  on  intracellular  tree 
calcium  in  isolated  and  fura-2  loaded  smooth-muscle  cells  from  the 
anterior  byssus  retractor  (catch)  muscle  of  Mytilns  din/is.  Pflugers 
Archiv4\4:  162-170. 

Kirber.  M.  T.,  J.  V.  Walsh,  Jr.,  J.  J.  Singer.  1988.  Stretch-activated 
ion  channels  in  smooth  muscle:  a  mechanism  for  the  initiation  of 
stretch-induced  contraction.  Pflugers  Arc/uv  412:  339-345. 

Mandelbaum.  D.  E.  1980.  Studies  of  the  role  of  cyclic  AMP  and  protein 
phosphor}  lation  in  the  mediation  of  food  arousal  in  Aplysia.  Ph.D. 
thesis.  Columbia  University.  154  pp. 

Morris,  C.  E.,  and  W.  J.  Sigurdson.  1989.  Stretch-inactivated  ion 
channels  coexist  with  stretch-activated  ion  channels.  Science  243: 
807-809. 

Ram,  J.  I..  1983.  Neuropeptide  activation  of  an  identifiable  buccal 
ganglion  motoneuron  in  Aplysia.  Brain  Res  288:  177-186. 

Ram,  J.  L.,  G.  S.  Ajimal,  D.  Gole,  K.  A.  Haller,  and  A.  Williams. 
1984a.  Serotonin  and  forskolin  enhance  both  magnitude  of  con- 
traction and  relaxation  rate  of  Aplysia  dorsal  extrinsic  muscle  in- 
dependently of  acetylcholine  receptor.  Comp.  Biochem.  Physio/.  79C: 
455-459. 

Ram,  J.  L.,  S.  Barmatoski,  L.-X.  Liu,  and  S.  C.  Mahaffey. 
1990.  Structure,  contraction,  and  electrophysiological  properties  of 
intact  and  dissociated  smooth  muscle  fibers  of  Aplysia.  Pp.  65 1-658 
in  Frontiers  in  Smooth  Muscle  Research  Progress  in  Clinical  and 
Biological  Research.  Vol.  327.  N.  Sperelakis  and  J.  D.  Wood.  eds. 
Wilev-Liss.  New  York. 


Ram,  J.  L.,  D.  Gole,  V.  Shukla.  and  I..  Greenberg.  1983.  Serotonin- 
actuated  adenylate  cyclase  and  the  possible  role  of  cyclic  AMP  in 
modulation  of  buccal  muscle  contraction  in  Aplysia.  J  Neurobiol. 
14:  113-121. 

Ram,  J.  L.,  and  L.-X.  Liu.  1990.  Ion  channels  in  Aplysia  smooth  muscle: 
currents  activated  by  voltage  and  acetylcholine  (ACh).  Soc.  Netirosci. 
Abstr.  16:  675. 

Ram,  J.  L.,  and  R.  Parti.  1985.  Regulation  of  calcium  influx  into  buccal 
muscles  of  Aplysia  by  acetylcholine  and  serotonin.  /.  Neurobiol.  16: 
57-68. 

Ram.  J.  L.,  and  S.  Patel.  1989.  Skinned  muscle  fibers  of  Aplysia:  po- 
tentiation  of  contraction  by  "background"  calcium  and  lack  of  effect 
of  cyclic  AMP.  Comp.  Biochcm  Physiol.  93A:  745-749. 

Ram,  J.  L.,  U.  A.  Shukla,  and  G.  S.  Ajimal.  1981.  Serotonin  has  both 
excitatory  and  inhibitory  modulatory  effects  on  feeding  muscles  in 
Aplysia  J  Neurobiol.  12:61 3-62 1 . 

Ram,  J.  L.,  U.  A.  Shukla,  R.  Parti,  and  R.  L.  Goines. 
1984b.  Extracellular  calcium  dependence  of  contracture  and  mod- 
ulation by  serotonin  in  buccal  muscle  E 1  of  Aplysia.  J.  Neurobiol. 
15:  197-206. 

Reilly,  W.  M.,  and  B.  Peretz.  1987.  Excitation-contraction  coupling 
in  a  non-spiking  smooth  muscle  in  the  gill  of  Aplysia.  J  Comp.  Phys- 
iol. 157B:  659-666. 

Rosen,  S.  C.,  I.  kupfermann,  R.  S.  Goldstein,  and  K.  R.  Weiss. 
1983.  Lesion  of  a  serotonergic  modulatory  neuron  in  Aplysia  pro- 
duces a  specific  defect  in  feeding  behavior.  Brain  Res.  260:  1 50- 155. 

Rosen,  S.  C.,  K.  R.  Weiss,  R.  S.  Goldstein,  and  I.  Kupfermann. 
1989.  The  role  of  a  modulatory  neuron  in  feeding  and  satiation  in 
Aplysia.  effects  of  lesioning  of  the  serotonergic  metacerebral  cells.  J. 
Neurosci  9:  1562-1578. 

Sigurdson,  W  .  J.,  and  C.  E.  Morris.  1989.  Stretch-activated  ion  channels 
in  growth  cones  of  snail  neurons.  /  Neurosci  9:  2801-2808. 

Sigurdson,  W.  J.,  C.  E.  Morris,  B.  L.  Brezden,  and  D.  R.  Gardner. 
1987.  Stretch  activation  of  a  K+  channel  in  molluscan  heart  cells. 
J  Exp.  Bu>l.  127:  191-209. 

Sugi,  H.,  and  T.  Yamaguchi.  1976.  Activation  of  the  contractile  mech- 
anism in  the  anterior  byssal  retractor  muscle  of  Mytilns  edulis.  J. 
Physiol..  Land.  257:  531-547. 

Tameyasu,  T..  and  H.  Sugi.  1976.  Effect  of  acetylcholine  and  high  ex- 
ternal potassium  ions  on  4SCa  movements  in  molluscan  smooth 
muscle.  Comp.  Biochem  Physiol.  53C:  101-103. 

Twarog,  B.  M.,  and  Y.  Muneoka.  1972.  Calcium  and  the  control  of 
contraction  and  relaxation  in  a  molluscan  catch  muscle.  Cold  Spring 
Harbor  Symp.  Quant  Biol.  37:  489-503. 

Twarog,  B.  M.  1954.  Responses  of  a  molluscan  smooth  muscle  to  ace- 
tylcholine and  5-hydroxytryptamine.  /  Cell.  Comp.  Physiol.  44:  141- 
164. 

Weiss,  K.  R.,  J.  L.  Cohen,  and  I.  Kupfermann.  1978.  Modulatory  con- 
trol of  buccal  musculature  by  a  serotonergic  neuron  (metacerebral 
cell)  in  Aplysia.  J.  Neurophysiol.  41:  181-203. 

Wilkens,  L.  A.,  and  M.  J.  Greenberg.  1973.  Effects  of  acetylcholine 
and  5-hydroxytryptamine  and  their  ionic  mechanisms  of  action  on 
the  electrical  and  mechanical  activity  of  molluscan  heart  smooth 
muscle.  Comp.  Biochem.  Physio/.  45A:  637-651. 

Zhang,  F.,  and  J.  L.  Ram.  1990.  Serotonin  (5-HT)  potentiates  con- 
traction of  dissociated  fibers  of  Aplysia  smooth  muscle.  Soc.  Neurosci. 
Abstr.  16:  1227. 


Reference:  Biol.  Bull  180:  284-244.  (April,  1941) 


Photoresponsiveness  of  Aplysia  Eye  is  Modulated  by 
the  Ocular  Circadian  Pacemaker  and  Serotonin 


JON  W.  JACKLET 

Department  of  Biological  Sciences,  Neurobiology  Research  Center,  State  University 
of  New  York  at  Albany,  Albanv.  New  York  12222 


Abstract.  The  eye  of  the  sea  hare,  Aplysia,  contains  a 
circadian  pacemaker  that  controls  rhythmic  behaviors  of 
the  animal.  This  report  shows  that  the  pacemaker  controls 
the  photoresponsiveness  of  the  eye  as  well.  The  electro- 
retinogram  (ERG)  of  the  isolated  eye-optic  nerve  prepa- 
ration, evoked  by  brief  green  light  pulses  in  otherwise  dark 
conditions,  was  recorded  regularly,  while  the  circadian 
rhythm  of  compound  action  potential  activity  was  con- 
tinuously recorded  from  the  optic  nerve.  The  waveform 
of  the  ERG  changed  systematically  and  rhythmically  dur- 
ing the  circadian  cycle.  One  wave  component  of  the  ERG 
was  prominent  during  the  subjective  night  phase  of  the 
rhythm  when  the  compound  action  potential  frequency 
was  minimal;  and  it  was  inconspicuous  during  the  sub- 
jective day  phase  of  the  rhythm  when  the  compound  ac- 
tion potential  frequency  was  maximal.  Because  eyes  at- 
tached to  the  central  nervous  system  and  isolated  eyes 
both  exhibited  the  same  rhythmic  ERG  changes,  the  cir- 
cadian pacemaker  in  the  eye  is  responsible  for  modulation 
of  the  ERG.  Addition  of  serotonin,  a  putative  efferent 
transmitter,  to  the  bathing  saline  induced  the  ERG  wave 
component  characteristic  of  the  subjective  night  phase  of 
the  rhythm.  The  threshold  serotonin  concentration  was 
10  7  AI.  and  serotonin  had  a  long  lasting  effect. 

Introduction 

Each  eye  of  Aplysia  contains  a  circadian  pacemaker 
(Jacklet,  1969a)  that  produces  a  circadian  rhythm  in  the 
frequency  of  optic  nerve  (ON)  autonomous  compound 
action  potentials  (CAPs).  The  CAP  activity  is  produced 


Received  9  August  1990;  accepted  28  December  1990. 

Non-standard  abbreviations:  CAP,  compound  action  potential:  ON, 
optic  nerve:  CT,  circadian  time;  ERG,  electroretinogram;  CM.  culture 
medium. 


by  the  synchronous  firing  of  a  population  of  retinal  pace- 
maker neurons  (Jacklet  et  ai,  1982),  the  axons  of  which 
enter  the  optic  nerve  and  project  to  the  central  ganglia 
(Olson  and  Jacklet,  1985).  This  rhythm  of  CAP  activity 
is  known  to  control  rhythmic  behaviors,  such  as  loco- 
motor  activity-rest,  because  eyeless  animals  lack  the  well 
defined  circadian  rhythm  of  activity-rest  that  normal  an- 
imals display  (Strumwasser  et  at.,  1979;  Lickey  et  ai, 
1977).  Feeding  behavior  also  exhibits  a  circadian  rhythm 
(Kupfermann,  1974).  but  the  contribution  of  the  ocular 
circadian  pacemaker  to  that  rhythm  has  not  been  tested. 
The  ocular  circadian  pacemaker  probably  controls 
rhythmic  behavior  by  neural  connections  to  central  motor 
control  centers,  or  by  affecting  physiological  processes  in 
the  eye  itself,  although  the  mechanisms  are  not  yet  known. 

An  eye  contains  the  photoreceptors  needed  to  entrain 
the  ocular  circadian  rhythm  to  the  solar  day  light-dark 
cycles,  because  the  rhythm  is  entrained  by  light-dark  cycles 
(Eskin.  1971),  and  the  phase  of  the  CAP  frequency  rhythm 
is  shifted  by  light  pulses  given  during  the  subjective  night, 
yielding  a  phase  response  curve  (Jacklet,  1974).  The  spe- 
cific ocular  photoreceptor  responsible  for  the  phase  shifts 
have  not  been  identified.  The  eyes  and  other  cephalic 
photoreceptors  also  mediate  simple  phototactic  behaviors 
(see  Jacklet,  1980). 

The  electroretinogram  (ERG)  has  been  recorded  from 
the  eye  by  an  extracellular  pipette  placed  in  the  retina 
(Jacklet,  1969b)  and  by  a  suction  electrode  placed  on  the 
cornea  (Eskin,  1977;  Eskin  and  Maresh,  1982).  The  ERG 
amplitude  is  increased  by  serotonin  treatment  or  by  ON 
stimulation,  which  presumably  releases  serotonin  from 
the  terminal  in  the  eye  of  the  efferent  neurons  (Eskin  and 
Maresh,  1982).  Eyes  contain  several  types  of  photorecep- 
tors, in  addition  to  the  pacemaker  neurons  involved  in 
the  circadian  rhythm.  The  largest  and  most  numerous 
type  is  the  R  photoreceptor  (Jacklet  and  Rolerson,  1982) 


284 


ORCADIAN  CONTROL  OF  PHOTORESPONSIVENESS 


285 


that  responds  to  light  with  a  graded  prolonged  depolar- 
ization. The  light  response  of  this  receptor  is  largely  re- 
sponsible for  the  ERG  (Jacklet,  1969b). 

Rhythmic  changes  in  the  ERG  ofAplysia.  or  any  other 
gastropod,  have  not  been  reported,  to  my  knowledge,  even 
though  ERG  circadian  rhythms  of  other  animals  are  well 
known.  For  example,  the  ERG  amplitude  rhythm  of  the 
compound  eye  of  Limit/us  has  been  intensively  studied 
(Barlow.  1983).  Photosensitivity  increases  by  20-100  fold 
during  the  night,  and  the  rhythm,  driven  by  a  central 
nervous  system  circadian  pacemaker,  is  mediated  by  ef- 
ferent innervation.  The  efferent  transmitter  appears  to  be 
octopamine  (Battelle  el  ai,  1989;  Kass  and  Barlow,  1984). 

I  report  here  that  the  ERG  waveform  recorded  from 
the  isolated  Aplysia  eye  changes  rhythmically,  and  the 
rhythm  maintains  a  stable  phase  relationship  with  the  cir- 
cadian rhythm  in  CAP  frequency.  Thus,  the  ocular  cir- 
cadian pacemaker  affects  physiological  processes  within 
the  eye  itself.  The  waveform  of  the  ERG  that  is  charac- 
teristic of  subjective  night  is  induced  by  the  addition  of 
serotonin  to  the  bathing  saline  during  subjective  day, 
mimicking  the  influence  of  the  circadian  pacemaker  dur- 
ing subjective  night. 

Materials  and  Methods 

Individuals  of  Aplysia  californica  were  obtained  from 
Marinus,  Inc.,  Long  Beach.  California,  and  kept  in  Instant 
Ocean  tanks  maintained  at  16°C  under  light-dark  cycles 
of  12:12.  Two  isolated  preparations  were  used:  the  eye- 
ON,  and  the  eye  attached  to  the  cerebral  ganglion  by  the 
ON.  They  were  placed  in  a  recording  dish  (100  ml) 
equipped  with  tubing  (polyethylene,  PE)  electrodes 
embedded  in  the  RTV  silicone  rubber  base.  Preparations 
were  maintained  in  a  dark  box  at  18°C  for  several  days 
of  recording.  The  ON  was  drawn,  by  negative  pressure 
applied  by  a  syringe,  into  one  electrode  (PE  10)  that  was 
used  to  record  CAPs,  and  the  eye  was  drawn  into  another 
electrode  (PE  50)  for  ERG  recording.  The  negative  pres- 
sure was  released,  and  the  eye  and  ON  remained  in  place 
for  recording.  The  eye  was  drawn  completely  into  the 
electrode  so  that  the  activity  of  all  retinal  cells  could  be 
recorded. 

The  eye  is  spherical  and  about  0.7  mm  in  diameter.  It 
has  a  central  lens,  a  poorly  developed  cornea,  and  a  com- 
plex retina  containing  photoreceptors  and  neurons  (see 
Jacklet  el  ai,  1982;  Herman  and  Strumwasser,  1984). 
ERG  electrode  recordings  routinely  picked  up  small  CAPs. 
Activity  was  amplified  with  an  A-M  Systems  model  1700 
(gain,  XI 000;  bandpass  0.1-1000  Hz.)  and  displayed  on 
a  Tektronix  5300  oscilloscope.  ERGs  and  CAPs  were  re- 
corded and  stored  with  Asystant+  software  in  an  IBM 
AT  computer,  and  the  data  were  plotted  with  a  Hewlett- 
Packard  model  7470A  plotter.  The  latencies  of  the  ERGs 


and  CAPs  were  measured  from  computer  generated  plots 
of  the  recordings. 

Pairs  of  eyes  from  eight  animals  were  used  in  the  cir- 
cadian rhythm  study.  Four  eyes  attached  to  the  cerebral 
ganglion  yielded  1 1  circadian  cycles  of  data,  and  7  isolated 
eyes  yielded  1 2  circadian  cycles  of  data.  Eyes  produced 
three  to  four  cycles  of  CAP  rhythm  data  routinely,  but 
complete  ERG  data  were  not  collected  from  all  eyes.  Pairs 
of  eyes  from  seven  animals  were  used  in  the  serotonin 
experiments. 

Light  pulses  were  produced  by  driving  an  Archer  green 
LED  with  1  -2  s  ( 1 5,  30  or  70V)  pulses  from  a  Grass  model 
S88  stimulator.  Pulses  were  given  at  regular  intervals  of 
10  min,  30  min,  or  1  h  in  otherwise  constant  darkness. 
The  LED  was  positioned  3  cm  away  and  directly  over  the 
eye  drawn  into  the  PE  tubing.  The  intensity  incident  on 
the  eye  was  measured  by  placing  the  sensor  of  a  radi- 
ometer/photometer (United  Detector  Corp.,  Model  40X) 
3  cm  from  the  LED.  Light  intensities  for  voltage  pulses 
used  to  drive  the  LED  were  0.6  ^W/cm2  at  1 5  V,  0.8  ^  W/ 
cm2  at  30  V,  and  3  ^W/cm2  at  70  V.  The  Aplysia  eye  has 
high  sensitivity  to  green  light  and  the  threshold  intensity 
is  about  0.06  ^W/cm2  at  500  nm  (Jacklet,  1980). 

Artificial  seawater  ( ASW)  was  made  up  of  the  following 
salts  in  millimoles/liter:  NaCl,  425;  KC1,  10;  CaCl:,  10; 
MgCl:,  22;  MgSO4,  26;  NaHCO,,  2.5;  adjusted  to  pH 
7.8.  Culture  medium  (CM)  was  composed  of  ASW,  20% 
Aplysia  blood,  and  100  U/ml  penicillin,  0. 1  mg/ml  strep- 
tomycin. Serotonin  (Sigma,  creatinine  sulfate)  was  added 
to  the  CM  to  final  concentrations  of  10  7,  10",  or  10  - 
M.  A  10-ml  chamber  fitted  with  polyethylene  tubing  for 
changing  solutions  inside  the  dark  box  was  used  for  the 
serotonin  experiments.  The  CM  was  removed  entirely  by 
applying  suction  to  the  polyethylene  tubing  and  was  re- 
placed within  a  few  seconds  with  the  serotonin  solution. 
The  statistically  significant  differences  between  average 
latencies  were  determined  using  a  two  tailed  /  test.  The 
level  of  significance  used  was  a  =  .05. 

Results 

Rhythmic  changes  in  the  ERG 

The  basic  ERG  waveform  recorded  in  these  experi- 
ments was  triphasic,  even  though  the  entire  eye  was  pulled 
into  the  tubing  electrode.  The  waveform  consisted  of  a 
sharply  rising  wave,  followed  by  a  slower  wave  of  opposite 
polarity  and  a  weak  slow  third  phase.  There  was  no  ob- 
vious "off"  response.  This  triphasic  ERG  waveform  is 
very  similar  to  the  ERG  recorded  by  Eskin  (1977)  on  a 
Grass  polygraph  using  a  suction  electrode  applied  on  or 
near  the  cornea. 

The  latency  of  the  ERG  in  the  present  study  was  about 
0.9  s  in  response  to  a  1  ^W/cm2  green  light  pulse.  This 
compares  well  with  the  latency  of  about  0.9  s  obtained 


286 


J.  W.  JACKLET 


3          sec. 


3          sec. 


1  -, 


a 
E 
to  - 


24 


48 


hr. 


50O-, 


hr. 


ORCADIAN  CONTROL  OF  PHOTORESPONS1VENESS 


287 


earlier  in  response  to  about  6  lux  (<1  /jW/cm:)  white 
light  (Jacklet,  1969b).  This  is  a  rather  long  latency,  but 
similar  to  those  of  other  gastropod  eyes  under  similar 
conditions.  Latency  is  1-3  s  for  Otala,  a  land  snail  (Ciliary 
and  Wolbarsht,  1967),  and  0.2-0.5  s  for  the  well-formed 
eye  of  Stronibu.s,  another  marine  gastropod  (Gillary. 
1974).  The  Aplysia  eye  latency  is  about  0.4  s  in  response 
to  600  lux  white  light  (Jacklet,  1969b). 

The  ERG  waveform  was  usually  smooth  during  the 
subjective  day,  as  shown  in  Figure  1 A  at  CT2.5.  CT  refers 
to  circadian  time,  which  is  measured  from  the  actual  pe- 
riod of  the  free-running  circadian  rhythm  of  interest,  in 
this  case  the  CAP  frequency  rhythm  shown  in  Figure  ID. 
The  period  is  divided  into  24  equal  units.  Circadian  time 
0-12  is  subjective  day,  and  CT  12-24  is  subjective  night. 
The  phase  point  in  the  CAP  rhythm  corresponding  to 
subjective  dawn  (CT  0)  is  the  CAP  frequency  at  '/:  max- 
imum marked  in  Figure  ID.  During  subjective  night,  the 
waveform  developed  a  notch  following  the  initial  wave, 
as  shown  in  Figure  1 A  at  CT  20.5.  For  convenience,  the 
waveform  has  been  labeled  in  the  figure.  The  initial  wave 
is  A,  followed  by  the  B-C  wave  or  notch,  followed  by  the 
slower  D  wave.  The  B-C  wave  has  not  previously  been 
reported.  This  waveform  is  typical  of  the  ERGs  recorded 
during  subjective  night  in  these  experiments. 

The  ERGs  shown  in  Figure  1 A  were  recorded  from  the 
same  isolated  eye  at  different  phases  (CT  20.5  and  CT 
2.5)  in  the  circadian  cycle.  Similar  ERGs  were  recorded 
from  the  other  eye  of  the  same  animal  at  the  same  phases, 
even  though  the  eye  was  attached  to  the  cerebral  ganglion 
by  the  ON  (Fig.  IB).  Being  attached  to  the  cerebral  gan- 
glion made  no  apparent  difference  in  the  waveform  of  the 
ERG  or  in  the  rhythmic  changes  in  the  waveform.  In 
general,  eye  pairs  exhibited  very  similar  ERG  waveforms, 
whether  or  not  the  eye  was  attached  to  the  cerebral  gan- 
glion. 

The  ERG  B-C  wave  changed  rhythmically  during  the 
circadian  cycle.  It  virtually  disappeared  during  subjective 
day  and  reappeared  during  subjective  night.  The  relative 
amplitude  of  the  B-C  wave,  measured  from  the  peak  of 
the  B  to  the  peak  of  the  C  wave,  cycled  continuously,  as 
shown  in  Figure  1C  for  the  B-C  wave  of  the  isolated  eye 


used  in  Figure  1  A.  When  the  cycling  of  the  relative  am- 
plitude of  the  B-C  wave  is  compared  to  the  CAP  frequency 
rhythm  plotted  in  Figure  ID,  the  maximum  B-C  wave 
amplitude  seems  to  occur  at  about  CT  20  and  to  coincide 
with  minimal  CAP  frequency  during  subjective  night.  The 
period  of  both  rhythms  is  about  24  hours. 

A  few  eyes  exhibited  weak  rhythmic  changes  in  the  A 
wave  of  the  ERG,  but  either  they  did  not  persist  over  two 
cycles,  or  they  were  not  sufficiently  robust  to  be  considered 
true  rhythms.  The  D  wave  was  very  stable  and  showed 
no  rhythmicity. 

The  ERG  waveform  recorded  from  eyes  of  different 
animals  varied  somewhat,  but  the  basic  waveform  could 
always  be  observed.  One  of  the  most  extreme  waveforms 
is  shown  in  Figure  2A,  B.  This  eye  was  attached  to  the 
cerebral  ganglion,  but  the  paired  isolated  eye  exhibited 
the  same  ERG  waveform  and  changed  rhythmically.  The 
A,  B,  C.  and  D  waves  are  readily  apparent.  But  the  A 
wave  is  relatively  small  and  the  B-C  wave  is  huge.  The 
relative  amplitude  of  the  B-C  wave  changed  rhythmically, 
but  it  never  completely  disappeared.  It  remained  in  the 
appropriate  phase  relationship  with  the  CAP  frequency 
rhythm  for  many  cycles  (Fig.  2C,  D).  The  period  of  both 
rhythms  is  about  23  hours. 

In  most  preparations,  the  latency  (time  from  stimulus 
onset  to  '/:  peak  of  A  wave)  of  the  ERG  A  wave  was  shorter 
during  the  subjective  night,  when  the  B-C  wave  was 
prominent,  than  during  the  subjective  day.  For  example, 
in  Figure  1A  and  B  the  latency  is  about  100  ms  shorter 
during  subjective  night.  The  ERG  latencies  for  light  pulses 
given  at  CT  19-22,  during  subjective  night,  were  com- 
pared to  latencies  obtained  at  CT  1-4,  during  subjective 
day,  for  1 2  circadian  cycles  for  both  isolated  eyes  and  eyes 
attached  to  the  cerebral  ganglion.  Mean  latency  and  the 
standard  error  of  the  mean  (SEM)  were  calculated,  and  / 
tests  were  performed  to  determine  whether  mean  differ- 
ences were  statistically  significant.  The  average  latency 
for  isolated  eyes  was  1000  ms  (SEM,  20;  N,  34)  during 
CT  19-22,  and  1050  ms  (SEM,  10;  N,  39)  during  CT  1- 
4.  The  means  were  significantly  different  at  the  .05  level. 
The  average  latency  for  eyes  attached  to  the  cerebral  gan- 
glion was  920  ms  (SEM,  20;  N,  36)  during  CT  19-22,  and 


Figure  1.  Rhythmic  changes  in  the  ERG.  ERG  waveforms  recorded  from  the  same  isolated  eye  during 
subjective  night  (CT  20.5)  and  during  subjective  day  (CT  2.5)  are  shown  in  A.  The  A.  B.  C.  and  D  waves 
are  labeled.  The  2-s  light  pulse  (3  /jW/cm2)  is  indicated  by  the  black  bar.  The  other  eye  from  the  same 
animal,  but  attached  to  the  cerebral  ganglion,  exhibited  the  ERG  waveform  shown  in  B  taken  at  CT  20.5 
and  CT  2.5  as  in  A.  Vertical  scales  in  A  and  B  are  50  /jV  and  25  ^V  per  division.  The  changes  in  relative 
amplitude  of  the  B-C  wave  of  the  ERGs  recorded  from  the  isolated  eye  are  plotted  in  C  using  the  same  time 
scale  as  the  CAP  frequency  rhythm  shown  in  D.  Arrows  identify  the  relative  amplitude  points  of  the  B-C 
wave  in  C,  and  the  CAP  frequency  in  D  corresponding  to  the  ERGs  in  panel  A  taken  at  CT  20.5  and  CT 
2.5.  Time  reference  for  CT  0  is  the  thin  labeled  line  in  D  that  occurs  at  'A  the  maximum  CAP  frequency. 
The  projected  light-dark  cycle  experienced  by  the  animal  before  dissection  is  shown  by  the  white/crosshatched 
bar. 


288 


J.  W.  JACK.LET 


CT  15 


sec. 


400 


ORCADIAN  CONTROL  OF  PHOTORESPONSIVENESS 


289 


970  ms  (SEM,  20;  N,  34)  during  CT  1  -4.  The  means  were 
significantly  different  at  the  .05  level.  Although  the  mean 
A  wave  latencies  are  shorter  for  attached  eyes  than  for 
isolated  eyes,  both  show  similar  shifts  in  ERG  latency 
during  the  circadian  cycle. 

To  test  for  the  involvement  of  chemical  synapses  in 
the  circadian  pacemaker  modulation  of  the  ERG  wave- 
form, two  eyes  were  subjected  to  ASW  containing  10  4 
M  Ca++  and  10  '  M  Mg++.  This  treatment  drastically 
reduced  the  ERG  as  expected  (Eskin,  1977).  Thus,  a  re- 
liable test  for  the  involvement  of  chemical  synapses  was 
not  possible. 

Removal  of  the  ON  from  the  isolated  eye  did  not  in- 
terrupt cycling  of  the  ERG  waveform,  but  it  did  reduce 
the  number  of  cycles  that  an  eye  exhibited.  The  ON  was 
cut  away  from  four  eyes  at  the  bases  of  their  retinas,  and 
ERGs  were  recorded  as  usual.  Small  CAPs  recorded  with 
the  ERG  electrode  verified  that  the  CAP  circadian  rhythm 
continued.  Two  of  the  eyes  remained  active  for  two  cycles, 
and  both  exhibited  cycling  of  the  B-C  wave,  suggesting 
that  the  ON  itself  is  not  necessary  for  circadian  cycling 
of  the  ERG. 

Serotonin  induces  the  ERG  B-C  wave 

Serotonin  induced  the  B-C  wave  in  eyes  at  circadian 
times  when  it  was  not  normally  expressed.  The  induced 
B-C  wave  closely  resembled  the  wave  characteristic  of 
subjective  night.  As  shown  in  the  example  of  Figure  3, 
the  B-C  wave  was  well  developed  at  CT  20,  as  expected, 
and  it  became  inconspicuous  later,  at  CT  0.5,  during  sub- 
jective day.  A  short  time  later  at  CT  2.0.  and  just  1 3  min 
after  the  addition  of  10~6  M  serotonin  to  the  bathing  so- 
lution by  perfusion,  the  induced  B-C  wave  (Fig.  3C)  was 
nearly  identical  to  the  B-C  wave  recorded  at  CT  20  (Fig. 
3A).  Continued  exposure  to  serotonin  enhanced  the  B-C 
wave  (Fig.  3D)  beyond  the  amplitude  of  the  subjective 
night  B-C  wave.  The  effects  of  serotonin  were  long  lasting 
and  reached  a  maximum  in  about  1  h.  Once  induced,  the 
B-C  wave  required  several  hours  of  washout  before  it  re- 
turned to  normal.  Serotonin  also  enhanced  the  B-C  wave 
of  eyes  tested  during  subjective  night  when  the  wave  was 
already  present. 

During  exposure  to  serotonin,  the  autogenous  CAP 
frequency  was  reduced  as  expected  from  previous  work 


(CorrenU'M/..  1978;  Eskin  and  Maresh,  1982).  The  num- 
ber of  CAPs  evoked  by  the  light  pulse,  especially  those 
evoked  several  seconds  after  the  pulse,  were  also  reduced 
as  shown  in  Figure  4B.  However,  compared  to  the  re- 
sponse just  before  the  addition  of  10~6  M  serotonin,  there 
was  no  change  in  the  latency  of  the  initial  CAP  produced 
by  a  light  pulse.  The  mean  latency  for  the  initial  CAP  in 
the  light  response  was  1.40  s  (SEM,  0.14;  N,  7)  before 
serotonin  treatment,  and  1 .36  s  (SEM,  0.05;  N,  7)  1 5  min 
after  serotonin  treatment.  A  /  test  showed  that  these  means 
were  not  significantly  different.  The  initial  CAP  occurs  at 
about  the  same  time  as  the  B-C  wave,  and  small  deflections 
on  the  ERG  waveform  caused  by  CAPs  are  clearly  visible 
(Fig.  4A,  B).  During  serotonin  treatment,  the  CAP  de- 
flections on  the  ERG  waveform,  as  well  as  the  size  and 
timing  of  the  initial  CAP  during  the  light  response,  re- 
mained the  same.  Thus,  changes  in  the  light-evoked  ac- 
tivity of  the  pacemaker  neurons  that  produce  the  CAPs 
are  not  likely  to  account  for  the  B-C  wave  induced  by 
serotonin. 

Serotonin  shortened  the  latency  of  the  ERG  and  oc- 
casionally increased  the  amplitude  of  the  A  wave.  Both 
Figures  3  and  4  show  a  substantial  decrease  in  the  latency 
and  increase  in  the  A  wave.  The  average  ERG  latency 
before  serotonin  was  960  ms  (SEM,  20;  N,  9);  15  min 
after  the  addition  of  10  6  M  serotonin  it  was  900  ms  (SEM, 
30;  N,  9),  and  45  min  after  an  addition  of  serotonin  it 
was  860  ms  (SEM  40;  N,  7).  The  difference  in  mean  la- 
tency was  not  significant  at  1 5  min,  but  at  45  min  it  was 
significantly  different  at  the  .05  level.  The  average  am- 
plitude of  the  A  wave  increased  only  1.1  times  at  45  min. 
At  the  threshold  concentration  (10  7  M)  for  ERG  B-C 
wave  induction,  the  average  latency  decreased  by  only  25 
ms  (N,  4),  and  the  A  wave  amplitude  did  not  change. 

Discussion 

ERG  waveform  changes 

A  major  finding  of  our  study  is  that  the  ERG  changes 
systematically  and  rhythmically  during  the  circadian  cycle 
of  CAP  frequency.  One  component  of  the  ERG,  the  B-C 
wave,  is  prominent  during  the  subjective  night  phase  of 
the  rhythm  when  the  CAP  rate  is  minimal,  and  incon- 
spicuous during  the  subjective  day  phase  when  the  CAP 


Figure  2.  Rhythmic  changes  in  the  ERG  of  an  eye  attached  to  the  cerebral  ganglion.  Waveforms  char- 
acteristic of  subjective  night  (CT  15)  and  subjective  day  (CT  3)  for  the  same  eye  attached  to  the  cerebral 
ganglion  by  the  ON  are  shown  respectively  in  A  and  B.  The  1-s  light  pulse  (3  /jW/cm2)  is  indicated  by  the 
black  bar.  Vertical  scales  in  A  and  B  are  50  j/V  per  division.  The  B-C  wave  became  very  prominent  in  the 
eyes  of  this  animal  after  several  days  of  recording.  C  shows  the  changes  in  relative  amplitude  of  the  B-C 
wave  plotted  on  the  time  scale  of  the  CAP  frequency  rhythms  in  D.  Arrows  in  C  and  D  mark  the  times  at 
which  the  ERGs  shown  in  A  and  B  were  taken.  Time  reference  for  CT  0  is  the  thin  labeled  line  in  D  at  '/: 
the  maximum  CAP  frequency.  The  projected  light-dark  cycle  experienced  by  the  animal  before  dissection 
is  shown  by  the  white/crosshatched  bar.  Light  pulse  is  indicated  by  black  bar  in  panels  A,  B. 


290 


J.  W.  JACKLET 


O 


B 


•ec. 


Figure  3.  Changes  in  the  ERG  of  an  isolated  eye  induced  by  serotonin.  The  ERG  waveform  recorded 
at  CT  20  with  a  characteristic  B-C  wave  is  shown  in  A.  At  CT  0.5  the  ERG  had  changed  to  the  subjective 
day  waveform,  lacking  the  B-C  wave.  Thirteen  min  after  the  addition  of 10~6  M  serotonin,  the  ERG  shown 
in  C  with  a  prominent  B-C  wave  was  recorded.  The  B-C  wave  progressively  increased  until  the  maximum 
response  in  D  was  observed  at  43  min  after  the  addition  of  serotonin.  At  that  time  the  latency  of  the  A  wave 
had  decreased  about  100  ms,  and  the  A  wave  amplitude  had  increased  140%.  Vertical  scale  is  20  »V  per 
division.  The  black  bar  on  the  time  scale  marks  the  1-s  light  (.8  nW/cnr)  pulse. 


rate  is  maximal.  Thus,  the  maximal  B-C  wave  occurs  dur- 
ing minimal  CAP  rate  in  an  antiphase  relationship.  The 
B-C  wave  decreases  in  size  sharply  during  the  increase  in 
CAP  rate  at  subjective  dawn,  suggesting  that  a  high  CAP 
rate  might  suppress  the  B-C  wave.  However,  the  causal 
relationship  between  these  events  has  not  been  deter- 


mined. Isolated  eyes,  and  eyes  attached  to  the  cerebral 
ganglion,  exhibited  similar  ERG  rhythms  suggesting  that 
the  B-C  wave  pacemaker  resides  within  the  eye  and  that 
it  is  likely  to  be  the  CAP  circadian  pacemaker.  The  in- 
duction of  the  B-C  wave  by  serotonin  is  an  intriguing 
observation  that  will  be  addressed  later  in  this  discussion. 


ORCADIAN  CONTROL  OF  PHOTORESPONSIVENESS 


291 


> 

3 

o 


sec. 


sec. 


Figure  4.  Changes  in  the  ERG  and  CAP  frequency  induced  by  serotonin.  Simultaneous  recordings  were 
made  of  CAPs  (upper  traces)  and  the  ERG  (lower  trace)  in  response  to  1-s  light  pulses  (dark  bar).  Responses 
at  CT  1.5  appear  in  A.  CAPs  appear  on  the  ON  trace  and  as  small  deflections  on  the  ERG  trace  as  well.  At 
CT  2.5,  43  min  after  addition  of  10~6  M  serotonin,  the  B-C  wave  seen  in  B  was  prominent,  the  latency  had 
decreased  about  100  ms,  and  the  number  of  CAPs  evoked  decreased.  Black  bars  on  time  scale  mark  the  1- 
s  light  (.8  MW/cnr)  pulse.  Vertical  scales  are  50  /W  per  division. 


292 


J.  W.  JACKLET 


The  ERG  B-C  wave  characteristic  of  subjective  night 
has  not  been  previously  reported.  Several  years  ago,  I 
looked  for  rhythmic  changes  in  the  ERG  A  wave  ampli- 
tude because  ERG  amplitude  changes  are  well  known  in 
other  animals  (see  Barlow,  1983),  but  did  not  find  con- 
sistent rhythms.  In  the  present  study,  the  use  of  computer 
assisted  recording  to  compare  ERG  waveforms  soon  re- 
vealed that  the  B-C  wave  occurs  and  changes  rhythmically. 
It  also  revealed  that  the  A  wave  latency  changes  rhyth- 
mically, but  that  the  A  wave  amplitude  does  not.  Average 
latency  differences  of  50  ms  during  the  cycle  were  found 
for  both  isolated  and  attached  eyes. 

The  ERG  recordings  were  made  from  eyes  completely 
pulled  into  the  tubing  electrode,  so  signals  could  be  re- 
corded from  any  of  the  responding  cell  types  in  the  eye, 
including  cells  in  the  basal  retina.  Previous  work  had 
shown  that  the  largest  photoresponses,  recorded  with  a 
glass  pipette  in  the  retina,  were  corneal  negative  and  were 
obtained  near  the  distal  segments  of  the  large  photore- 
ceptors  (Jacklet.  1969b).  In  the  present  study,  light  from 
the  LED  reached  all  surfaces  of  the  isolated  eye  and  was 
not  restricted  to  the  pathway  through  the  cornea  and  lens. 

The  photoreceptor  organization  of  the  Aplysia  retina 
was  investigated  with  localized  illumination  by  Block  and 
McMahon  (1983).  They  illuminated  (100  lux,  white  light) 
the  distal  segments  of  the  photoreceptors  surrounding  the 
lens  and,  as  a  result,  unitary  ON  activity  without  CAPs 
was  evoked  in  the  ON.  Illumination  of  the  basal  retina 
produced  CAPs.  Block  and  McMahon  concluded  that 
chemical  synaptic  inhibition,  especially  the  inhibitory  ac- 
tion of  the  receptor  layer  onto  the  CAP  generating  neu- 
rons, shapes  in  part  the  light  responses  of  the  isolated  eye. 
Other  evidence  of  synaptic  inhibition  in  the  retina  is  pro- 
vided below. 

Retinal  cells  that  may  contribute  to  the  ERG  B-C  wave 

The  ERG  consists  of  a  sharply  rising  A  wave,  a  rhyth- 
mically changing  B-C  wave,  and  a  stable  D  wave.  The  A 
wave  is  likely  to  be  caused  by  the  R  type  photoreceptors 
with  microvillous  distal  segments  adjacent  to  the  lens 
(Jacklet.  1969b;  Jacklet  and  Rolerson,  1982).  Intracellular 
recordings  show  that  dark  adapted  R  photoreceptors  re- 
spond to  white  light  pulses  of  600  lux  after  a  latency  of 
400  ms,  comparable  to  the  ERG  latency  of  400  ms  at  that 
intensity  (Jacklet,  1969b);  the  response  is  a  prolonged  de- 
polarization of  60-70  mV  (Jacklet  and  Rolerson,  1982). 
Light  adapted,  but  not  dark  adapted,  R  photoreceptors 
have  a  notch  on  the  rising  phase  of  the  depolarization. 
The  notch  is  probably  not  responsible  for  the  B-C  wave 
because  it  occurs  early  during  the  A  wave,  and  because 
ERGs  were  recorded  at  interstimulus  intervals  of  up  to  1 
h  when  the  R  photoreceptor  are  dark  adapted  and  not 
expected  to  have  a  notch.  Some  R  photoreceptors  display 


prolonged  hyperpolarization  following  the  initial  depo- 
larization (see  Fig.  3  in  Jacklet  and  Rolerson.  1982). 
However  this  hyperpolarization  is  also  unlikely  to  cause 
the  B-C  wave  because  it  continues  much  longer  than  the 
B-C  wave.  Responses  from  R  photoreceptors  have  not 
been  studied  throughout  the  circadian  cycle,  especially 
not  during  the  subjective  night  when  the  B-C  wave  occurs, 
so  changes  that  might  account  for  the  B-C  wave  have  not 
been  observed. 

Light  responses  of  R  photoreceptors  are  not  completely 
blocked  by  low  Ca++  and  high  Mg*  *  ASW,  but  the  resting 
potential  is  decreased,  and  the  light-evoked  depolarization 
is  reduced  and  prolonged  (Jacklet  and  Rolerson,  1982). 
This  should  account  for  the  reduction  of  the  ERG  in  low 
Ca+4  and  high  Mg++  ASW  previously  observed  (Eskin, 
1977)  and  confirmed  in  this  study. 

The  pacemaker  neurons  (or  secondary  neurones.  Jack- 
let  et  a/.,  1982)  responsible  for  the  CAPs  also  respond  to 
light.  They  depolarize  and  fire  synchronous  action  poten- 
tials that  are  correlated  1 : 1  with  the  ON  CAPs.  As  shown 
in  this  study.  CAPs  produce  small  but  observable  deflec- 
tions on  the  ERG  waveform  at  all  phases  of  the  circadian 
cycle,  but  they  are  tiny  compared  to  the  B-C  wave,  and 
none  are  synchronized  with  the  B-C  wave.  The  light  re- 
sponses of  the  pacemaker  neurons  themselves  seem  un- 
likely to  contribute  to  the  B-C  wave.  However,  the  B-C 
wave  is  most  conspicuous  during  the  phase  of  the  circadian 
cycle  when  the  autogenous  CAP  frequency  is  low  or  ab- 
sent. Perhaps  low  autogenous  CAP  activity  creates  the 
conditions  necessary  for  the  B-C  wave  to  occur.  The  re- 
lationship between  autogenous  CAP  activity  and  expres- 
sion of  the  B-C  wave  has  not  yet  been  tested  directly. 

A  retinal  cell  type  that  may  contribute  to  the  ERG  B- 
C  wave  is  the  H  photoreceptor  (Jacklet  and  Rolerson, 
1982).  Its  typical  light  response  is  a  volley  of  action  po- 
tentials followed  by  brisk  hyperpolarization,  and  then  de- 
polarization accompanied  by  action  potentials.  The  hy- 
perpolarization occurs  just  after  the  initial  photoresponse, 
at  about  the  time  that  the  B-C  wave  of  the  ERG  is  oc- 
curring. The  H  cell  hyperpolarization  appears  to  be  syn- 
aptically  evoked,  because  electrical  stimulation  of  the  ON 
evokes  a  similar  sharp  hyperpolarization  (Jacklet  and 
Rolerson,  1982).  This  cell  type  may  be  involved  in  shaping 
the  light  responses  observed  by  Block  and  McMahon 
(1983)  during  selective  illumination.  The  ERG  B-C  wave 
might  be  produced  by  enhanced  inhibitory  synaptic  in- 
teractions within  the  retina  controlled  by  the  circadian 
pacemaker.  Such  enhanced  interactions  might  improve 
the  visual  performance  of  the  eye. 

A  determination  of  the  retinal  cell  types  in  Aplysia  that 
contribute  to  the  rhythmic  B-C  wave  must  await  a  sys- 
tematic intracellular  study  of  cellular  light  responses 
throughout  the  circadian  cycle,  preferably  with  simulta- 
neous ERG  recordings. 


ORCADIAN  CONTROL  OF  PHOTORESPONSIVENESS 


293 


The  eye  of  a  marine  gastropod.  Stwmhux,  may  share 
some  of  the  features  of  the  Aplysia  eye  photoresponses, 
including  the  B-C  wave.  The  ERG  exhibits  two  peaks  of 
negativity  that  are  separable  under  certain  conditions  of 
light  and  temperature  (Gillary,  1974).  The  second  peak 
resembles  the  B-C  wave.  This  eye  is  3  times  the  diameter, 
and  contains  about  100  times  as  many  cells,  as  the  Aplysia 
eye.  It  appears  to  lack  circadian  pacemaker  neurons,  and 
changes  in  the  photoresponse  during  the  circadian  cycle 
have  not  been  explored  to  my  knowledge.  This  retina 
contains  two  types  of  depolarizing  cells  and  one  hyper- 
polarizing  type  (Quandt  and  Gillary,  1979),  similar  to  the 
Aplysia  retina.  One  depolarizing  cell  type  (Type  II)  exhibits 
two  peaks  of  depolarization  that  are  similar,  but  of  op- 
posite polarity,  to  the  ERG  waveform  (Quandt  and  Gil- 
lary, 1980). 

Role  of  serotonin 

Serotonin  has  been  shown  by  Eskin  and  Maresh  (1982) 
to  increase  the  first  wave  (A  wave  in  this  study)  of  the 
Aplysia  ERG  when  it  is  recorded  with  a  suction  electrode 
applied  to  the  cornea.  They  did  not  see  a  B-C  wave.  That 
may  be  due  to  differences  in  recording  methods,  because 
they  made  polygraph  recordings  and  did  not  report  laten- 
cies. They  found  an  average  increase  in  the  ERG  ampli- 
tude of  63%  after  20  min  in  10~6  M  serotonin,  and  a 
threshold  concentration  near  10~7  M.  Dopamine,  acetyl- 
choline,  and  octopamine  were  tested  but  did  not  produce 
consistent  ERG  changes.  They  also  reported  a  20%  in- 
crease in  the  ERG  in  response  to  ON  stimulation  and 
proposed  that  the  stimulation  might  cause  the  release  of 
serotonin  from  efferent  terminals  in  the  eye.  Terminals 
have  been  identified  by  serotonin  antisera  (Goldstein  el 
al.  1984;  Takahashi  el  ai,  1989). 

The  effect  of  serotonin  on  the  ERG  suggests  that  cyclic 
nucleotide  second  messengers  may  be  involved.  Cyclic 
AMP  mediates  many  of  the  serotonin  effects  on  short- 
and  long-term  central  synapses  in  Aplysia  (Kandel  and 
Schwartz,  1982),  and  serotonin  phase  shifts  the  CAP 
rhythm  (Corrent  et  al,  1978)  by  a  mechanism  involving 
cAMP  (Eskin  et  ai,  1982).  In  addition,  cGMP  mimics 
the  effect  of  light  on  the  circadian  pacemaker  by  inducing 
phase  shifts  of  the  CAP  rhythm  (Eskin  et  ai,  1984).  During 
the  cGMP  treatment,  the  membrane  potentials  of  R  pho- 
toreceptors  were  not  altered,  but  changes  in  photorespon- 
siveness  were  not  explored.  A  ten  minute  exposure  to  light 
increased  the  cGMP  level  by  50%..  Cyclic  GMP  may  be 
elevated,  either  as  a  consequence  of  photoresponses,  or 
because  it  is  involved  in  the  phototransduction  process 
(Eskin  el  ai,  1984).  If  any  rhythmic  changes  in  the  levels 
of  cyclic  nucleotides  occur,  they  might  be  involved  in  the 
B-C  wave  and  the  A  wave  latency  changes. 

Serotonin  does  not  appear  to  induce  the  B-C  wave  by 
a  direct  effect  on  the  initial  light  response  of  pacemaker 


neurons,  because  the  initial  CAP  that  most  nearly  coin- 
cides temporally  with  the  B-C  wave  is  unaffected  by  se- 
rotonin. Thus,  serotonin  does  not  allow  expression  of  the 
B-C  wave  by  suppressing  the  light-induced  pacemaker 
neuron  activity.  However,  the  phase  of  the  circadian  cycle 
during  low  or  zero  CAP  frequency  is  associated  with 
expression  of  the  B-C  wave,  and  serotonin  does  suppress 
autogenous  CAP  activity.  Serotonin  may  create  the  nec- 
essary conditions  for  expression  of  the  B-C  wave,  in  part, 
by  suppressing  autogenous  CAP  activity. 

Because  serotonin  induces  the  ERG  B-C  wave  and  the 
reduction  of  the  A  wave  latency,  one  may  ask  how  it  might 
be  involved  in  circadian  control  of  the  ERG.  Serotonin 
may  just  be  mimicking  a  natural  process,  but  because 
there  are  efferent  synaptic  terminals  containing  serotonin 
in  the  eye,  they  may  well  be  involved.  Because  isolated 
eyes  show  circadian  rhythms  in  the  B-C  wave,  the  control 
of  serotonin  release  from  the  efferent  terminals  by  central 
neurons  is  eliminated.  But  how  then  might  serotonin  be 
released  by  activity  within  the  eye?  Could  processes  con- 
trolled by  the  circadian  pacemaker  in  the  eye  release  se- 
rotonin? Because  the  appearance  of  the  B-C  wave  is  as- 
sociated with  minimal  CAP  frequency,  release  cannot  be 
a  direct  effect  of  CAP  activity.  To  produce  the  appropriate 
response,  autogenous  CAP  activity  would  have  to  suppress 
serotonin  release,  and  inactivity  would  have  to  promote 
release.  Otherwise  another  process  controlled  by  the  cir- 
cadian pacemaker  must  be  involved. 

Acknowledgments 

I  thank  Mark  Goldberg  for  excellent  technical  assis- 
tance. Research  supported  by  NSF  grant  BNS  88-19773 
toJ.W.J. 

Literature  Cited 

Barlow,  R.  B.,  Jr.  1983.  Circadian  rhythms  in  the  Limulus  visual  sys- 
tem. J.  Neumsci.  3:  856-870. 

Battelle,  B-A.,  J.  A.  Evans,  and  S.  C.  Chamberlain.  1982.  Efferent 
fibers  to  Limulus  eyes  synthesize  and  release  octopamine.  Science 
216:  1250-1252. 

Block,  G.,  and  D.  McMahon.  1983.  Localized  illumination  of  the 
Aplysia  and  Bulla  eye  reveals  new  relationships  between  retinal  layers. 
Brain  Res  265:  134-137. 

Corrent,  G.,  D.  McAdoo,  and  A.  Eskin.  1978.  Serotonin  shifts  the  phase 
of  the  circadian  rhythm  from  the  Aplysia  eye.  Science  202:  977-979. 

Eskin,  A.  1971.  Properties  of  the  Aplysia  visual  system:  in  vitro  en- 
trainment  of  the  circadian  rhythm  and  centrifugal  regulation  of  the 
eye.  Z  vergl.  Physiologic  74:  353-371. 

Eskin,  A.  1977.  Neurophysiological  Mechanisms  involved  in  photo- 
entrainment  of  the  circadian  rhythm  from  the  Aplysia  eye.  J.  Neu- 
robtoi  8:  273-299. 

Eskin,  A.,  and  R.  Maresh.  1982.  Serotonin  or  electrical  optic  nerve 
stimulation  increases  the  photosensitivity  of  the  Aplysia  eye.  Comp. 
Biochem.  Physiol.  73C:  27-31. 

Eskin,  A.,  J.  S.  Takahashi,  M.  Zatz,  and  G.  D.  Block.  1984.  Cyclic 
guanosine  3':5'-monophosphate  mimics  the  effects  of  light  on  a  cir- 
cadian pacemaker  in  the  eye  of  Aplysia.  J  Neurosci.  4:  2466-2471. 


294 


J.  W.  JACKLET 


Eskin,  A.,  G.  Corrent,  C.  Y.  Lin,  and  J.  McAdoo.  1982.  Mechanism 
for  shifting  the  phase  of  a  circadian  rhythm  by  serotonin:  involvement 
of  cAMP.  Proc.  Nat.  Acad,  Sci.  USA  79:  660-664. 

Ciliary,  H.  L.  1974.  Light-evoked  electrical  potentials  from  the  eye 
and  optic  nerve  of  Stromhus:  response  waveform  and  spectral  sen- 
sitivity. J.  Exp.  Bioi  60:  383-396. 

Ciliary,  H.  L.,  and  M.  L.  Wolbarsht.  1967.  Electrical  responses  from 
the  eye  of  a  land  snail.  Rev.  Can.  Biol  26:  125-134. 

Goldstein,  R.,  H.  B.  Kistler,  H.  W.  M.  Steinbusch,  and  J.  H.  Schwartz. 
1984.  Distribution  of  serotonin-immunoreactivity  in  juvenile 
Aplysia.  J.  Neurosci.  11:  535-547. 

Herman,  K.  G.,  and  F.  Strumwasser.  1984.  Regional  specializations  in 
the  eye  of  Aplysia.  a  neuronal  circadian  oscillator.  J.  Camp.  Neural. 
230:  593-613. 

Jacklet,  J.  W.  1969a.  Circadian  rhythm  of  optic  nerve  impulses  re- 
corded in  darkness  from  isolated  eye  of  Aplysia.  Science  164:  562- 
563. 

Jacklet,  J.  W.  1969b.  Electrophysiological  organization  of  the  eye  of 
Aplysia.  J.  Gen.  Physio/.  53:  21-42. 

Jacklet,  J.  \V.  1974.  The  effect  of  constant  light  and  light  pulses  on 
the  circadian  rhythm  in  the  eye  of  Aplysia.  J  Comp.  Physio/  90: 
33-45. 

Jacklet,  J.  \V.  1980.  Light  sensitivity  of  the  rhinophores  and  eyes  of 
Aplysia.  J.  Comp.  Physiol.  136:  257-262. 

Jacklet,  J.  W.,  and  C.  Rolerson.  1982.  Electrical  activity  and  structure 
of  retinal  cells  of  the  Aplysia  eye.  II.  Photoreceptors.  J.  Exp.  Bio/ 
99:381-395. 


Jacklet,  J.  W.,  L.  Schuster,  and  C.  Rolerson.  1982.     Electrical  activity 

and  structure  of  retinal  cells  of  the  Aplysia  eye.  I.  Secondary  neurones. 

/  Exp.  Biol.  99:  369-380. 
kandel,  E.  R.,  and  J.  H.  Schwartz.  1982.     Molecular  biology  of  learning: 

Modulation  of  transmitter  release.  Science  218:  433-443. 
Kass,  L.,  and  R.  B.  Barlow,  Jr.  1984.     Efferent  neurotransmission  of 

circadian  rhythms  in  Limulus  lateral  eye.  J  Neurosci.  4(4):  908-9 1 7. 
kupfermann.  I.  1974.     Feeding  behavior  of  Aplysia:  A  simple  system 

for  the  study  of  motivation.  Behav.  Biol  10:  1026. 
Lickey,   M.,   J.   Wozniak,   G.   Block,   D.   Hudson,   and   G.   Augter. 

1977.     The  consequences  of  eye  removal  for  the  circadian  rhythm 

of  behavioral  activity  in  Aplysia.  J.  Comp.  Physiol  118:  121-143. 
Olson,  L.,  and  J.  W.  Jacklet.  1985.     The  circadian  pacemaker  in  the 

eye  of  Aplysia  sends  axons  throughout  the  central  nervous  system. 

J.  Neurosci.  5(12):  3214-3227. 
Quandt,  F.  N.,  and  H.  L.  Gillarj.  1979.     Classes  of  light-evoked  response 

in  the  retina  of  Stmmhus.  J.  Exp.  Biol.  80:  287-297. 
Quandt,  F.  N.,  and  H.  L.  Ciliary.  1980.     Separable  phases  of  light-evoked 

depolarizations  in  the  retina  of  Slrombus.  J  E.\p  Biol  84:  137-148. 
Strumwasser,  F.,  R.  Alvarez,  D.  Viele,  and  J.  Woolum.  1979.     Structure 

and  function  of  a  circadian  oscillator  system.  Pp.  5 1  -56  in  Biological 

Rhythms  and  their  Central  Mechanisms.  M.  Suda,  O.  Hayaishi.  and 

H.  Nakagawa.  eds.  Elsevier/North  Holland  Biomedical  Press. 
Takahashi,  J.  S.,   D.   E.  Nelson,  and   A.   Eskin.    1989.     Immuno- 

cytochemical  localization  of  serotonergic  fibers  innervating  the  ocular 

circadian  system  of  Aplysia.  Neuroscience  28:  139-147. 


Reference:  Bwl  Bull.  180:  295-300.  (April,  1991) 


Control  of  Central  and  Peripheral  Targets  by  a 
Multifunctional  Peptidergic  Interneuron 

DAVID  J.  PRIOR 

Department  of  Biological  Sciences,  Northern  Arizona  University,  Flagstaff,  Arizona  86011 


Abstract.  In  the  terrestrial  slug.  Limax  maximus.  feed- 
ing activity  and  cardiovascular  function  have  been  shown 
to  be  correlated.  For  example,  in  intact  animals,  both 
feeding  responsiveness  and  heart  activity  are  suppressed 
during  dehydration  (Grega  and  Prior,  1986).  The  paired 
peptidergic  buccal  ganglion  neurons  RBI  and  LB1  have 
dramatic  modulatory  effects  on  both  the  feeding  motor 
program  (FMP)  and  the  force  of  heart  contraction  (Wels- 
ford  and  Prior,  1991).  The  Bl  neurons  appear  to  contain 
the  small  cardioactive  peptides  (SCPs).  Observations  have 
a  frequency  dependent  excitation  of  both  the  FMP  and 
the  heart  demonstrated  by  intracellular  stimulation  of  B 1 . 
Thus,  interneuron  Bl  may  serve  to  mediate  the  coincident 
modulation  of  multiple  responses  to  physiological  stresses. 

Introduction 

Environmental  stress  or  a  change  in  the  physiological 
state  of  an  organism  very  often  results  in  a  concerted  array 
of  regulatory  responses.  Such  responses  usually  include 
modification  of  behavioral  patterns,  or  the  level  of  be- 
havioral responsiveness,  as  well  as  changes  in  physiological 
functions  such  as  cardiac  output  and  respiratory  activity. 
With  the  use  of  certain  invertebrate  organisms,  recent  re- 
search has  addressed  the  question  of  the  control  of  such 
concerted  response  patterns  (Prior,  1989;  Teyke  et  al, 
1990;  Frugal  and  Brownell,  1987). 

Terrestrial  gastropods,  such  as  Limax  maximns,  are 
remarkably  susceptible  to  environmental  stresses  such  as 
dehydration.  In  a  drying  environment,  they  can  lose  30- 
40%  of  their  body  weight  within  a  few  hours  (see  Prior  et 
al.,  1983;  Riddle,  1983;  Prior,  1985,  for  reviews).  Among 
the  array  of  regulatory  responses  displayed  by  dehydrating 
slugs  are  contact-rehydration  (Prior,  1984;  Prior  and  Ug- 
lem,  1984),  modifications  in  respiratory  function  (Dick- 
Received  14  December  1990;  accepted  30  January  1991. 


inson  et  al.,  1988).  alterations  in  feeding  responsiveness 
(Prior,  1983;  Phifer  and  Prior.  1985)  and  modifications 
in  cardiovascular  function  (Grega  and  Prior,  1986;  Wels- 
ford  and  Prior,  199 1 ).  As  such,  Umax  represents  a  useful 
model  for  the  analysis  of  the  integration  of  multiple  reg- 
ulatory responses. 

The  concerted  control  of  feeding  behavior  and  cardio- 
vascular function  in  Umax  has  been  a  focus  of  recent 
work  (see  Grega  and  Prior,  1985;  Prior  and  Welsford, 
1989).  Rhythmic  feeding  behavior  in  this  organism  in- 
volves alternating  protraction  and  retraction  of  the  toothed 
radula  against  a  food  source.  Feeding  bouts  often  last 
many  minutes  and  can  involve  hundreds  of  bite  cycles 
(see  Gelperin  et  al..  1978).  In  semi-intact  or  isolated  prep- 
arations of  the  central  nervous  system  (CNS:  Fig.  1), 
chemical  stimuli  applied  to  the  lips  or  electrical  stimu- 
lation of  the  lip  nerves  can  elicit  a  prolonged  pattern  of 
efferent  neural  activity  that  underlies  the  feeding  move- 
ments. This  feeding  motor  program  (FMP;  Prior  and  Gel- 
perin, 1977;  Gelperin  et  al..  1978)  consists  of  alternating 
bursts  of  activity  in  protractor  and  retractor  motoneurons 
(Fig.  1,  2).  In  addition  to  activation  of  the  major  buccal 
musculature,  the  FMP  involves  synchronized  activation 
of  the  accessory  salivary  system.  During  feeding,  the  ac- 
tivity of  the  fast  salivary  burster  neurons  (FSBs),  which 
are  the  motoneurons  to  the  salivary  ducts,  becomes  phase- 
locked  with  protraction  (Fig.  2). 

SCPB  Modulation  of  Feeding  and  Heart  Function 

In  gastropods,  the  small  cardioactive  peptides  (SCPs) 
have  an  excitatory  effect  upon  both  the  musculature  (see 
Lloyd  and  Willows,  1988;  Lloyd,  1989)  and  the  neural 
networks  underlying  patterned  feeding  activity  (see  Wil- 
lows et  al..  1988).  In  several  species,  SCPB  can  initiate 
patterned  efferent  activity  in  isolated  CNS  preparations 
(e.g..  Helisoma.  Murphy  et  al..  1985;  Tritonia,  Willows 


295 


296 


D    J    PRIOR 


.SN 


BUCCAL 
GANGLION 


TENTACULAR 
NERVE  - 

CEREBRAL 
GANGLION 

ABDOMINAL 
GANGLION 


o   o 

MCG          MCG 


BODY  WALL 


Figure  1  .  A  diagram  of  the  isolated  central  nervous  system  of  Limax 
including:  the  paired  huccal  ganglia  and  the  fused  cerebral  and  abdominal 
ganglia;  the  pneumostome  region;  abdominal  nerves  N8-N12  and  the  pos- 
terior pedal  nerves,  PPN;  buccal  nerves  N  1-N3;  gastric  nerve,  GN;  salivary 
nerve.  SN;  buccal  protractor  motoneuron  B7;  fast  salivary  burster  neuron, 
SB;  cerebrobuccal  connective,  CBC;  metaccrebral  giant  cell.  MCG. 


ci  nl.,  1988).  In  Limax,  however,  SCPB  has  a  modulatory 
role,  increasing  the  responsiveness  of  the  central  pattern 
generator  to  stimuli  (Prior  and  Watson,  1987).  In  the 
presence  of  10  7  to  10  6  A/  SCPB,  otherwise  ineffective 
stimuli  can  initiate  full  expression  of  the  feeding  motor 
program. 

Among  the  neurons  in  Limax  that  are  responsive  to 
SCPB  are  the  paired  fast  salivary  bursters.  The  rate  of  en- 
dogenous burst  activity  in  these  motoneurons  is  enhanced 
by  application  of  SCPB  in  a  concentration-dependent 
manner  (Fig.  3.  4).  Short-term  application  ofSCPB  results 
in  a  slow  increase  in  FSB  burst  frequency  and  an  even 
slower  decrement  of  the  effect  follows  initiation  of  a  saline 
wash.  In  addition,  continuous  perfusion  of  a  preparation 
for  20-30  min  reveals  no  indication  of  desensitization  of 
the  effect.  In  10  6  M  SCPB.  the  burst  frequency  was  sus- 
tained at  14  bursts/min  compared  with  a  control  fre- 
quency of  1  burst/min  (see  Prior  and  Watson.  1987).  It 
has  been  determined  that  this  excitatory  effect  is  mediated 
by  an  increase  in  the  rate  of  the  interburst  depolarization 
rather  than  a  general  decrease  in  resting  potential  (Hess 
and  Prior,  1989).  Thus  the  effects  of  SCPB  on  the  Limax 
feeding  system  include  modulation  of  the  responsiveness 
of  the  FMP  in  addition  to  direct  excitation  of  specific 
motoneurons. 

To  assess  further  the  potential  role  of  an  SCPB-like  pep- 
tide  in  the  regulation  of  feeding  responsiveness,  exogenous 


LSN 


*•••••    I.I    I     .       ,        . 


B 


,                           lu|                  ,,      ^u| 

I                  i-i 

i      U  i 

II                     11,                     11 

yP»   '         'PI1 

1     Ik 

.  1    Ik 

Figure  2.  Activation  of  the  feeding  motor  program  (FMP)  in  an  isolated  buccal  ganglia-brain  preparation  by 
electrical  stimulation  of  an  external  lip  nerve  (artifacts  at  beginning  at  A).  The  FMP  is  characterized  by  alternation 
of  efferent  bursts  correlated  with  protraction  (buccal  nerve  1:  RBR1;  and  the  right  and  left  salivary  nerves:  RSN. 
LSN),  and  retraction  (buccal  nerve  2:  RBR2).  The  nonfeeding  endogenous  bursts  of  the  right  FSB  are  noted  with 
dots.  The  upper  calibration  trace  indicates  one  mark/second.  (From  Prior  and  Watson,  1987) 


MULTIFUNCTIONAL  PEPTIDERGIC  INTERNEURON 


297 


I   I  I  I  I 

t 


I     I   I  UNI 


SCPB  ON 


2. 


MINIMUM  I  I  Ml  I  I  M 


SCP_    OFF 

D 


3.  I     I     M      I      I     I     I      (      I     I     I     I     I      I      I     I      I      I      I      I. 


B 

LSB 


IL 


I 


ll 


T 

SCP    ON 

D 

Figure  3.  (A)  A  continuous  extracellular  recording  from  the  left  salivary  nerve  (LSN)  of  an  isolated 
buccal  ganglia-brain  preparation  is  shown  in  1-3.  The  prominent  bursting  unit  in  this  record  is  the  fast 
salivary  burster  (FSB;  each  burst  consists  of  12-15  spikes).  Within  20  s  of  the  application  of  2  x  10~6  M 
SCPB  to  the  preparation  (first  arrow),  the  burst  frequency  of  the  FSB  increases.  Following  removal  of  SCPB 
from  the  superfusion  medium  (second  arrow),  burst  frequency  of  the  FSB  returns  to  the  pretreatment  level. 
(B)  an  intracellular  recording  from  the  fast  salivary  burster  neuron  (FSB)  showing  the  increase  in  burst 
frequency  and.  in  this  case,  progressive  depolarization,  in  response  to  2  •  I(T6  M  SCPB  (the  dashed  line 
indicates  the  level  of  the  interburst  hyperpolarization  before  exposure  to  SCPB).  Bar  =  30  s  (A)  and  20  mV 
(B).  (From  Prior  and  Watson.  1988) 


SCPB  was  injected  into  intact  animals  and  their  feeding 
responsiveness  measured.  As  shown  in  Table  I.,  SCPB  can 
initiate  the  apetetive  phases  of  feeding  behavior  including: 
(1)  cessation  of  locomotion,  (2)  tentacular  retraction,  (3) 
lip  eversion,  and  (4)  lip  movement.  That  the  consumatory 
phase  of  feeding  was  not  regularly  initiated  was  not  un- 
expected, in  that  in  isolated  CNS  preparations  SCPB  did 
not  initiate  feeding,  but  rather,  increased  responsiveness 
to  stimuli.  Nevertheless,  this  would  appear  to  be  the  first 
demonstration  of  an  orderly  effect  of  injected  SCPB  in  an 
intact  organism.  This  result  certainly  supports  the  notion 
that  an  SCPB-like  peptidergic  system  is  involved  in  the 
control  of  the  feeding  system  in  Limax. 

The  small  cardioactive  peptides  have  been  shown  to 
have  an  excitatory  effect  on  the  musculature  of  numerous 
systems,  including  Helix  heart  (Lloyd  1978,  1982),  Aplysia 
and  Tritonia  buccal  mass  and  gut  (Lloyd  ct  a/.,  1984; 
Lloyd  and  Willows,  1988),  and  Limax  ventricle  (Welsford 
and  Prior,  1991;  Lloyd,  1979;  1989).  In  Limax,  both  SCPB 


and  SCPA  cause  a  concentration-dependent  increase  in 
the  force  of  ventricular  contraction  (Welsford  and  Prior, 
1991).  At  a  concentration  of  10  6  M,  SCPB  can  cause  a 
150%  increase  in  the  force  of  ventricular  contractions. 
Although  lower  concentrations  of  SCPB  ( 10~9  to  10"7  M) 
can  cause  a  slight  increase  in  heart  rate,  there  does  not 
appear  to  be  a  consistent  effect  (Prior  and  Welsford,  1 989). 
The  excitatory  effects  of  SCPB  on  heart  and  the  feeding 
system  of  Limax,  together  with  the  stress-induced  coin- 
cident changes  in  feeding  and  cardiovascular  function  ob- 
served in  intact  animals  (Grega  and  Prior,  1985),  are  in- 
dicative of  the  possibility  of  coincident  control  of  these 
two  systems. 

Multifunctional  Modulatory  Interneuron  Bl 

In  that  exogenous  SCPB  can  simultaneously  modify 
feeding  and  cardiovascular  function,  immunohistochem- 
ical  techniques  were  used  in  an  effort  to  identify  central 


298 


D.  J.  PRIOR 


•—  •2XIO~*M 
O  —  O  2XIO"7M 
A—  £  2XIO"* 


I.OX  SALINE 


456 

TIME(min) 


10 


Figure  4.  The  responses  of  the  fast  salivary  burster  neuron  (FSB)  to 
van  ing  concentrations  of  SCPB  are  presented  by  plotting  burst  frequency 
as  a  function  of  time  during  the  experiment.  In  each  case,  SCPB  was 
superfused  over  an  isolated  buccal  ganglion-brain  preparation  between 
minutes  2  and  4.  The  preparation  was  superfused  with  saline  for  20  min 
between  each  trial.  (A)  The  responses  obtained  in  three  trials  with  the 
same  preparation  using  various  concentrations  of  SCPB  are  shown.  Each 
point  represents  the  burst  frequency  of  the  FSB  in  the  preceding  60  s. 
(B)  The  responses  of  a  second  preparation  to  SCPB.  In  this  case  four 
different  concentrations  of  SCPB  were  used  as  well  as  a  control  saline 
trial.  (C)  The  extent  of  the  variability  between  preparations  is  illustrated 
by  plotting  the  mean  (±SD)  burst  frequency  at  each  time  point  for  29 
trials  in  12  preparations  during  exposure  to  2  x  10'6  A/  SCPB.  (From 
Prior  and  Watson,  1987) 


neurons  containing  SCPB-like-immunoreactive-material 
(SLIM)  that  might  be  involved.  Among  the  most  prom- 
inent SLIM-reactive  neurons  were  the  right  and  left  Bl 
buccal  neurons  (Prior  and  Watson,  1987).  In  addition  to 
those  neurons  that  clearly  contain  SCPB  immunoreactive 
material,  there  are  numerous  cell  bodies  that  are  enmeshed 
by  networks  of  immunoreactive  fibers  (e.g.,  B7,  FSB), 
which  is  suggestive  of  peptidergic  endings  near  the  target 
feeding  neurons. 

The  morphology  of  Bl  was  examined  by  intracellular 
injection  of  horseradish  peroxidase  (Fig.  5a).  There  are 


Figure  5a.  A  photomicrograph  of  a  preparation  of  paired  buccal 
ganglia  (outlined)  showing  the  morphology  of  the  left  Bl  neuron  injected 
with  horseradish  peroxidase.  Cerebrobuccal  connective.  CBC;  salivary 
nerve.  SN;  this  preparation  was  made  by  K.  Delaney) 


two  major  axonal  projections  and  an  extensive  dendritic 
arborization  in  the  lateral  lobe  of  the  buccal  ganglion.  A 
small  axon  projects  across  the  buccal-buccal  commissure. 


4 


Figure  5b.  Panel  1  is  a  camera  lucida  drawing  of  the  soma  of  LB1 
following  injection  of  Co**  showing  the  major  axon  exiting  the  buccal 
ganglion  in  the  ipsilateral  cerebrobuccal  connective.  Panel  2  is  a  camera 
lucida  drawing  of  the  abdominal  ganglion  showing  the  continued  axonal 
projection  of  the  injected  LB1,  with  one  axonal  branch  in  abdominal 
ganglion  nerve  9  and  two  axonal  branches  in  nerve  1 1 .  Panel  3  illustrates 
antidromic  activation  of  Bl  in  response  to  repetitive  stimulation  of  the 
cardiac  branch  of  nerve  9.  Panel  4  shows  repetitive  intracellular  stimu- 
lation of  Bl  causing  in  a  constant-latency  axonal  impulse  recorded  in 
nerve  9. 


MULTIFUNCTIONAL  PEPTIDERGIC  INTERNEURON 


299 


lahlc  I 


Behavioral  effects  iifSCPg  injeclums  in  Limax  maximus 


Behavioral  observations 


Treatment 

Tentacular  locomotion 

Lip  retraction 

Lip  eversion 

Lip  movement 

Saline 

83.0% 

8.0% 

0.0% 

0.0% 

ICT'moir1  SCPB 

83.0% 

25.0% 

17.0% 

8.0% 

lO-'moll'1  SCPB* 

92.0% 

33.0% 

42.0% 

25.0% 

ID"5  moir1  SCPB" 

17.0% 

83.0% 

100% 

58.0%- 

The  percentage  of  animals  that  displayed  each  behavior  is  presented  in  each  case. 
Each  animal  received  injections  of  each  concentration  of  SCPB  and  the  saline  control. 

The  0.05  probability  level  was  accepted  as  significant  (determined  by  a  Friedman's  test  and  a  non-parametric  multiple  comparisons  procedure). 
All  concentrations  of  SCPB  are  the  calculated  final  hemolymph  concentrations. 

The  results  with  1  5  and  10""  moll"'  SCPB  injection  were  significantly  different  from  those  with  injection  of  control  saline  and  10~7  moll~'  SCPB. 
Furthermore,  the  results  with  10~5  moll'1  SCPB  injection  were  significantly  greater  than  those  observed  with  injection  of  10~6  moll"'  SCPB. 
*  P  <  0.05,  **  P  <  0.01.  n  =  12. 
(From  Schagene  el  ai.  1989) 


Although  the  dendritic  arborizations  occur  primarily  in 
the  lateral  lobe,  they  do  span  into  the  medial  lobe,  in- 
cluding the  region  containing  both  retractor  and  protractor 
feeding  motoneurons.  The  major  axon  projects  out  the 
ipsilateral  cerebrocuccal  connective,  through  the  cerebral 
ganglion,  into  the  abdominal  ganglion  and  out  abdominal 


o 

^   350 
o 

UJ 

o: 
u. 

i-    250- 

<s> 

cc 

ID 

m 

m    150 
oo 


o 


o 


50- 


-50 


01       23456789 
B1  SPIKE  FREQUENCY  (Hz) 

Figure  6.  A  summary  of  the  change  in  the  burst  frequency  of  the 
fast  salivary  burster  neuron  initiated  by  intracellular  stimulation  ot  the 
ipsilateral  Bl  neuron  at  different  impulse  frequencies.  The  apparent 
threshold  frequency  for  Bl  is  2-4  Hz  with  the  maximal  effect  occurring 
at  about  7  Hz.  Changes  in  the  FSB  burst  frequency  were  normalized  as 
a  percentage  of  the  pre-stimulation  level  of  activity  in  each  preparation. 
Each  bar  represents  the  mean  (±SD)  response  of  five  buccal  ganglion- 
brain  preparations.  (From  Prior  and  Welsford.  1989) 


nerves  9  and  1 1,  which  innervate  the  heart  and  kidney 
complex,  respectively  (Fig.  5b).  Rapid  stimulation  (5 Hz) 
of  a  cardiac  branch  of  nerve  9  resulted  in  antidromic  ac- 
tivation of  the  soma  of  Bl.  Correspondingly,  repetitive 
activation  of  the  soma  of  Bl  by  intracellular  current  in- 
jection was  followed  by  a  constant  latency  impulse  in  the 
cardiac  branch  of  nerve  9  (Fig.  5a). 

The  immunohistochemical  results  together  with  the 
basic  morphology  of  Bl.  including  significant  arboriza- 
tions in  the  region  of  the  feeding  neurons,  and,  remark- 
ably, a  major  axonal  projection  to  the  heart,  were  sugges- 
tive of  a  role  for  Bl  in  the  concerted  control  of  feeding 
and  cardiovascular  functions. 

Intracellular  stimulation  of  Bl  at  quite  low  frequencies 
results  in  a  progressive  increase  in  the  activity  of  the  fast 


o 
cc 
o 

L_ 

z 
o 

1 

z 
o 
u 


o 
z 


I 
o 


75- 

c 

65- 

c 

c 

T 

c 

55- 
45- 
35- 
25- 
15- 
5- 

c 

ab 

i 

b 

I 

I 

I 

i 

1 

i 

1 

2345678 
B1  SPIKE  FREQUENCY  (Hz) 


10 


Figure  7.  The  effect  of  unilateral  stimulation  of  Bl  on  the  force  of 
ventricular  contraction.  The  bars  represent  the  mean  (±SD)  response 
from  56  trials.  Ten  action  potentials  were  elicited  at  each  frequency.  The 
a.  b,  c  notation  refers  to  significant  differences  (e.g..  C's  are  significantly. 
P  <  0.001,  different  from  a's  and  b's).  (Redrawn  from  data  of  Welsford 
and  Prior.  1991) 


300 


D.  J.  PRIOR 


salivary  bursters.  As  shown  in  Figure  6,  driving  Bl  at  5 
impulses/s  can  result  in  a  50%  increase  in  FSB  burst  fre- 
quency. Even  two  to  three  impulses  at  low  frequencies 
are  sufficient  to  elicit  a  transient  increase  in  FSB  burst 
frequency.  These  effects  are  sustained  in  high  Mg+  +  .  high 
Ca++  saline  indicating  the  possibility  of  monosynaptic 
connection. 

To  assess  the  potential  role  of  Bl  in  the  control  of  heart 
function,  semi-intact  preparations  of  the  CNS  and  inner- 
vated heart  were  used,  which  allowed  intracellular  stim- 
ulation of  Bl  and  measurement  of  ventricular  activity. 
Stimulation  of  Bl  at  low  frequencies  resulted  in  an  in- 
crease in  the  force  of  contraction  of  the  heart  (Fig.  7).  It 
is  of  interest  that  Bl  frequencies  of  5  to  7  impulse/s  were 
the  most  effective  in  the  activation  of  both  the  FSB  and 
the  heart. 

When  this  experiment  was  repeated  with  the  CNS 
bathed  in  high  Mgff,  high  Ca++  saline,  there  was  no 
change  in  the  effectiveness  of  Bl  to  increase  heart  function. 
This  suggests  that  B 1  has  a  direct  effect  on  peripheral  tar- 
gets rather  than  acting  via  additional  CNS  neuronal  path- 
ways. 

Thus,  it  would  appear  that  buccal  neuron  Bl  may  be 
a  multifunction  peptidergic  interneuron  capable  of  si- 
multaneously modulating  the  central  feeding  motor  pro- 
gram and  cardiovascular  function.  As  such,  Bl,  along  with 
other  similar  neurons,  is  positioned  to  control  the  syn- 
chrony of  multiple  behavioral  responses  normally  ob- 
served in  response  to  environmental  stress  and  changes 
in  the  physiological  state  of  an  organism. 

Acknowledgments 

The  work  described  in  this  paper  was  supported,  in 
part,  by  grants  from  the  Arizona  Disease  Control  Research 
Commission  (#82-0698)  and  The  National  Institutes  of 
Health  (M.B.R.S.*  2  SO3  RR03401-03). 

Literature  Cited 

Dickinson,  P.  S.,  D.  J.  Prior,  and  C.  Avery.  1988.  The  pneumostome 
closure  rhythm  in  slugs:  a  response  to  dehydration  controlled  by 
hemolymph  osmolality  and  peptide  hormones.  Comp.  Biochcm. 
Physiol.  89A(4):  579-586. 

Furgal,  S.  M.,  and  P.  H.  Brownell.  1987.  Ganglionic  circulation  and 
its  effects  on  neurons  controlling  cardiovascular  functions  in  the 
Aplysia  calilonuca  J  Exp.  Zool.  244:  347-364. 

Gelperin,  A.,  J.  J.  Chang,  and  S.  Reingold.  1978.  Feeding  motor  pro- 
gram in  Limax  maximus:  neuromuscular  correlates  and  control  by 
chemosensory  input.  J.  Neurobiol.  9:  285-300. 

Grega,  D.  S.,  and  D.  J.  Prior.  1985.  The  effects  of  feeding  on  heart 
activity  in  the  terrestrial  slug.  Limax  maximus:  central  and  peripheral 
control.  J.  Comp.  Physio/.  156A:  539-545. 

Grega,  D.  S.,  and  D.  J.  Prior.  1986.  Modification  of  heart  activity  in 
response  to  dehydration  in  the  terrestrial  slug.  Limax  maximus.  J 
Exp.  Biol.  237:  185-190. 


Hess,  S.  D.,  and  D.  J.  Prior.  1989.  Small  cardioactive  peptide  B  mod- 
ulates feeding  motoneurons  in  Limax  maximus  J  Exp.  Biol.  142: 
473-478. 

Lloyd,  P.  E.  1978.  Distribution  and  molecular  characteristics  ot  car- 
dioactive peptides  in  the  snail.  Helix  aspersa.  J.  Comp.  Physiol.  128: 
269-276. 

Lloyd,  P.  E.  1979.  Central  peptide  containing  neurons  modulate  gut 
activity  in  Trilona.  Soc.  Neurosci.  Abstr.  5:  252. 

Lloyd,  P.  E.  1982.  Cardioactive  neuropeptides  in  gastropods.  Fed.  Proc. 
41:2948-2852. 

Lloyd,  P.  E.  1989.  Peripheral  actions  of  the  SCPs  in  Aplysia  and  other 
gastropod  molluscs.  Am.  /.ool.  29:  1265-1274. 

Lloyd,  P.  E.,  I.  Kiiplri  m. urn,  and  K.  R.  Weiss.  1984.  Evidence  for 
parallel  actions  of  a  molluscan  neuropeptide  (SCPA)  and  serotonin 
in  mediating  arousal  in  Aplysia  Proc.  Nat.  Acad.  Sci.  L'SA  81:  2934- 
2937. 

Lloyd,  P.  E.,  and  A.  O.  D.  Willows.  1988.  Multiple  transmitter  neurons 
in  Tritimia  II.  Control  of  gut  motility.  /  \eurohml.  19(1):  55-67. 

Murphy,  A.  D.,  K.  Lukowiak,  and  \V.  K.  Stell.  1985.  Peptide  modu- 
lation of  patterned  motor  activity  in  identified  neurons  of  Hclisoma. 
Proc  Nat.  Acad  Sci.  USA  82:  7140-7144. 

Phifer,  C.  B.,  and  D.  J.  Prior.  1985.  Body  hydration  and  haemolymph 
osmolality  affect  feeding  and  its  neural  correlate  in  the  terrestrial 
gastropod,  Limax  maximus.  J.  Exp.  Biol.  1 18:  405-426. 

Prior,  D.  J.  1983.  Hydration-induced  modulation  of  feeding  respon- 
siveness in  terrestrial  slugs.  J.  Exp  Zool.  227:  15-22. 

Prior,  D.  J.  1984.  Analysis  of  contact-rehydration  in  terrestrial  gastro- 
pods: osmotic  control  of  drinking  behaviour.  J  Exp.  Biol  111:  63- 
73. 

Prior,  D.  J.  1985.  Water-regulatory  behaviour  in  terrestrial  gastropods. 
Biol.  Rer.  60:  403-424. 

Prior,  D.  J.,  and  G.  L.  Uglem.  1984.  Analysis  of  contact-rehydration 
in  terrestrial  gastropods:  absorption  of  l4C-inulin  through  the  epi- 
thelium of  the  foot. ./.  Exp.  Biol.  Ill:  73-80. 

Prior,  D.  J.  1989.  Neuronal  control  of  osmoregulatory  responses  in 
gastropods.  Pp.  1-24  in  Advances  in  Comparative  Environmental 
Physiology.  R.  Gilles,  ed.  5.  Springer- Verlag,  New  York. 

Prior,  D.  J.,  and  A.  Gelperin.  1977.  Autoactive  molluscan  neuron: 
reflex  function  and  synaptic  modulation  during  feeding  in  the  ter- 
restrial slug,  Limax  maximus  J.  Comp.  Physiol.  114:  217-232. 

Prior.  D.  J.,  and  W.  H.  Walson.  1987.  The  molluscan  neuropeptide, 
SCPB,  increases  the  responsiveness  of  the  feeding  motor  program  of 
Limax  maximus.  J.  Neurobiol.  19(1):  87-105. 

Prior,  D.  J.,  and  I.  G.  Welsford.  1989.  The  role  of  small  cardioactive 
peptide.  SCPB.  in  the  regulatory  responses  of  terrestrial  slugs.  Am. 
Zool.  29:  1255-1263. 

Prior,  D.  J.,  M.  Hume,  D.  Varga,  and  S.  D.  Hess.  1983.  Physiological 
and  behavioural  aspects  of  water  balance  and  respiratory  function 
in  the  terrestrial  slug,  Limax  maximus.  J.  Exp.  Biol.  104:  1 1 1-127. 

Riddle,  W.  A.  1983.  Physiological  ecology  of  land  snails  and  slugs.  Pp. 
431-461  in  The  Mollusca,  W.  D.  Russell-Hunter,  ed.  Ecology.  6. 
Academic  Press,  New  York. 

Schagene,  K.  A.,  I.  G.  Welsford,  D.  J.  Prior,  and  P.  A.  Banta. 
1989.  Behavioral  effects  of  injection  of  small  cardioactive  peptide, 
SCPB,  on  the  slug  Limax  maximus.  J  Exp.  Biol.  143:  553-557. 

Teyke,  T.,  K.  R.  Weiss,  and  I.  kupfermann.  1990.  An  identified  neuron 
(CPR)  evokes  neuronal  responses  reflecting  food  arousal  in  Aplysia. 
Science  247:  85-87. 

Welsford,  I.  G.,  and  D.  J.  Prior.  1991.  Modulation  of  heart  activity  in 
the  terrestrial  slug  Limax  maximus  by  the  feeding  motor  program, 
small  cardioactive  peptides  and  stimulation  of  buccal  neurone  Bl. 
./.  Exp.  Biol.  155:  1-19. 

Willows,  A.  O.  D.,  P.  E.  Lloyd,  and  B.  P.  Masinovsky.  1988.  Multiple 
transmitter  neurons  in  Trilonia  III.  Modulation  of  central  pattern 
generator  controlling  pattern.  /  Neurobiol.  19(  1 ):  69-86. 


Reference:  Binl.  Bull  180:  301-309.  (April,  1991) 


Opioid  Systems  and  Magnetic  Field  Effects 
in  the  Land  Snail,  Cepaea  nemomlis 

MARTIN  KAVALIERS  AND  KLAUS-PETER  OSSENKOPP 

Division  of  Oral  Biology,  Faculty  of  Dentistry,  and  Department  of  Psychology, 
University  of  Western  Ontario,  London.  Ontario,  Canada  N6A  5C1 


Abstract.  Accumulating  evidence  shows  that  magnetic 
fields  can  affect  a  variety  of  opioid-mediated  behavioral 
and  physiological  functions.  The  idea  that  endogenous 
opioids  are  involved  in  the  mediation  of  fundamental  be- 
havioral responses  in  invertebrates  is  also  gaining  support. 
Evidence  exists  for  opioid  involvement  in  the  mediation 
of  nociceptive  and  antinociceptive  ("analgesic")  responses 
of  the  land  snail,  Cepaea  neinoralis.  and  other  mollusks. 
in  a  manner  comparable  to  that  in  vertebrates.  Exposure 
to  various  magnetic  stimuli,  including  weak  60  Hz  mag- 
netic fields,  has  significant  inhibitory  effects  on  exogenous 
opiate-induced  analgesia  and  endogenous  opioid-me- 
diated nociceptive  responses  of  Cepaea  in  a  manner  anal- 
ogous to  that  described  for  vertebrates.  These  effects  of 
the  magnetic  stimuli  are  evident  under  both  laboratory 
and  natural  conditions  and  include  disruptions  of  the  day- 
night  rhythms  of  opioid-mediated  nociception.  These 
similar  effects  in  Cepaea  and  rodents  raise  the  possibility 
of  a  phylogenetic  continuity  in  the  effects  of  magnetic 
fields  on  basic  opioid-mediated  biological  responses. 

Introduction 

Results  of  field  and  laboratory  studies  show  that  the 
behavioral,  cellular,  and  physiological  functions  of  ani- 
mals can  be  affected  by  magnetic  stimuli  (see  reviews  in 
Adey,  1981;  Gould,  1984;  Ossenkopp  and  Kavaliers, 
1988).  These  diverse  actions  have  led  to  speculation  on 
the  possible  modes  of  action  of  magnetic  fields  on  bio- 
logical systems  (Leask,  1977;  Semm  et  al.,  1980;  Adey, 
1 98 l;Kirschvink  and  Gould,  1981;  Liburdy  et  al..  1987; 
Liboff  and  McLeod,  1988;  Blackman  et  al..  1989). 

Evidence  has  accumulated  that  endogenous  opioid  sys- 
tems and  opioid  peptides,  which  are  involved  in  the  mod- 
Received  19  July  1990;  accepted  6  November  1990. 


ulation  of  a  broad  range  of  basic  functions  (Akil  et  at.. 
1984),  can  be  affected  by  magnetic  stimuli.  Substantial 
data  now  indicates  that  time-varying  magnetic  fields,  es- 
pecially those  in  the  extremely  low  frequency  (ELF)  range 
(0.10-100  Hz),  affect  endogenous  opioid  systems  and  the 
actions  of  exogenous  opiates  such  as  morphine  (Kavaliers 
and  Ossenkopp,  1984,  1986.  1987;  Miller  el  al.,  1985; 
Ossenkopp  and  Kavaliers.  1987;  Praloetal.,  1987).  Opioid 
systems  may,  thus,  be  an  integral  part  of  the  mechanism(s) 
whereby  magnetic  fields  exert  their  diverse  behavioral  and 
physiological  effects  (Ossenkopp  and  Kavaliers,  1988). 

Although  interest  has  primarily  focused  on  vertebrates, 
there  is  evidence  that  magnetic  fields  affect  a  variety  of 
behavioral  physiological  processes  in  invertebrates  (Gould, 
1984).  Recently,  opioid-mediated  behaviors  that  are  sen- 
sitive to  magnetic  stimuli  have  been  demonstrated  in  a 
gastropod  mollusk,  the  land  snail  Cepaea  neinoralis 
(Kavaliers  et  al..  1983;  Kavaliers  and  Ossenkopp,  1989). 
This  paper  briefly  describes  (i)  opioid  modulation  of  be- 
havioral responses  in  mollusks  and  (ii)  the  effects  of  mag- 
netic fields  on  opioid  mediated  responses  and  their  day- 
night  rhythms  in  the  snail,  Cepaea. 

Opioid  Systems  and  Molluscs 

General  aspects 

In  vertebrates,  endogenous  opioid  peptides  co-exist  with 
diverse  hormones  in  endocrine  glands  and  with  classical 
or  peptide  transmitters  in  peripheral  autonomic  and  sen- 
sory neurones.  In  addition,  opioid  peptides  are  widely 
distributed  in  the  central  nervous  system  where  they 
function  as  transmitters  or  neuromodulators.  Three  fam- 
ilies of  endogenous  opioid  peptides  derived  from  three 
precursor  peptides  are  known  to  date:  the  pro-opiomela- 
nocortin  (POMC),  the  pro-enkephalin,  and  the  pro-dy- 
norphin  system.  These  precursors  undergo  differential 


301 


302 


M.   K.AVALIERS  AND  K..-P.  OSSENKOPP 


processing  in  various  regions  of  the  central  and  peripheral 
nervous  systems,  and  the  major  cleavage  products  have 
different  affinities  to  the  three  major  types  of  opioid  re- 
ceptors: n,  5,  and  K  (Hollt,  1986). 

These  opioid  peptides  and  receptors  have  now  been 
identified  in  a  variety  of  invertebrate  taxa,  strongly  sug- 
gesting a  phylogenetic  conservation  of  opioid  peptide 
structure  and  function  (Kream  el  a/.,  1980;  Leung  and 
Stefano,  1984,  1987;  Scharrer  el  a/..  1988;  Zisper  el  al.. 
1988;  Leung  et  al..  1990;  Santoro  el  al..  1990).  Results  of 
behavioral,  electrophysiological,  immunological,  and 
pharmacological  studies  have  shown  that  endogenous 
opioid  peptides  and  exogenous  opiate  agonists  and  an- 
tagonists have  behavioral  and  physiological  actions  in  in- 
vertebrates resembling  those  induced  in  mammals  (Ste- 
fano, 1982,  1989;  Leung  and  Stefano,  1987;  Stefano  el 
al..  1989). 

Behavioral  aspects 

Nociception.  One  of  the  primary  roles  of  vertebrate 
opioid  systems  is  the  modulation  of  nociception  and  be- 
havioral responses  to  aversive  and  stressful  stimuli  (Besson 
and  Chaouch,  1987;  Kavaliers,  1989a).  In  nature,  animals 
commonly  encounter  aversive  stimuli  that  can  influence 
their  survival.  To  effectively  respond  to  these  stimuli,  or- 
ganisms require:  (i)  a  mechanism  for  recognizing  aversive 
stimuli,  (ii)  a  set  of  effectors  that  can  react  to  the  noxious 
stimulus,  and  (iii)  a  system  for  producing  coordinated 
and  directed  movements  and  behavior  in  response  to  the 
stimuli.  The  ability  of  animals  to  recognize  and  physically 
react  to  aversive  or  noxious  stimuli  that  can  compromise 
their  integrity  is  embodied  in  the  term  "nociception" 
(Sherrington,  1906).  Nociceptors  are  preferentially  sen- 
sitive to  either  a  noxious  stimulus  or  to  an  aversive  stim- 
ulus that  would  become  noxious  if  prolonged,  and  they 
code  the  intensity  of  the  stimulus  (Besson  and  Chaouch, 
1987).  In  addition,  the  responses  from  the  effectors  are 
appropriate  to  the  input  from  the  receptors.  Nociception 
can  be  used  to  provide  an  index  of  an  animal's  sensitivity 
to  aversive  environmental  conditions  and,  thus,  can  allow 
for  the  determination  of  the  capacity  to  execute  adaptive 
behavior.  Measurements  of  alterations  in  nociceptive-re- 
lated  responses  (decreases  in  sensitivity-antinociception 
or  analgesia  when  considered  in  terms  of  pain)  are  widely 
used  to  determine  the  behavioral  and  physiological  status 
of  animals  following  exposure  to  aversive,  or  potentially 
aversive,  stimuli.  In  rodents,  laboratory  measures  of  no- 
ciception include  recording  of  limb  flexion  or  withdrawal 
(lifting  a  foot  off  an  aversive,  usually  thermal  surface); 
active  avoidance  (flinch  jump,  jumping,  or  moving  from 
an  aversive  situation);  and  removal  of  the  tail  away  from 
a  thermal  stimulus  (tail-flick)  (Kavaliers,  1989a). 

Assays  for  invertebrate  nociception  have  been  devel- 
oped, and  nociceptive  responses  have  been  observed  in 


invertebrates  as  well  as  vertebrates  (Kavaliers,  1989a).  For 
example,  within  a  few  seconds  after  Cepaea  is  placed  on 
a  surface  warmed  to  40°C,  the  snail  lifts  the  anterior  por- 
tion of  its  fully  extended  foot  away  from  the  aversive  sur- 
face (Fig.  1).  The  behavioral  end  point  used  is  the  time 
at  which  the  foot  reaches  its  readily  discernible  maximum 
elevation.  This  "foot-lifting"  behavior  is  not  observed  in 
snails  that  are  exposed  to  temperatures  normally  present 
in  their  natural  habitats,  but  becomes  increasingly  evident 
as  the  temperature  is  raised  towards  40°C.  This  nocicep- 
tive response  is  comparable  to  the  foot-lifting  response 
exhibited  by  rodents  when  placed  on  a  warmed  surface. 
Similar,  thermally  induced  nociceptive  responses  have 
also  been  reported  for  the  snail.  Helix  aspersa.  and  the 
slug,  Arion  alter  (Leung  and  Stefano,  1987;  Dalton  and 
Widdowson,  1989).  A  nociceptive  function  is  also  indi- 
cated for  specific  mechanoafferent  neurons  innervating 
the  tail,  parapodia,  and  much  of  the  foot  and  body  wall 
of  the  marine  mollusk  Aplysia  califarnica  (Walters  and 
Erickson,  1986).  These  neurons  display  increasing  dis- 
charge frequency  in  response  to  progressively  increasing 
pressure,  with  maximal  responses  occurring  to  stimuli  that 
could  cause  tissue  damage  (Walters,  1987).  A  similar 
graded  pattern  of  response  has  been  used  to  define  the 
activity  of  classical  mammalian  nociceptors  (Besson  and 
Chaouch,  1987). 

Opioid  mediation  of  nociception  and  antinociception. 
Antinociception  has  been  widely  documented  in  experi- 
mental animals  following  exposure  to  diverse  environ- 
mental stimuli,  with  both  opioid  and  non-opioid  mech- 
anisms being  implicated  (Rodgers  and  Randall,  1988). 
Furthermore,  it  now  seems  clear  that  environmentally  in- 
duced pain  inhibition  is  an  important  component  of  an 
organism's  defensive  repertoire  and  hence  has  high  adap- 


Figure  1.  Thermal  'nociceptive'  response  of  a  hydrated  individual 
Cepaea  nemoralis  placed  on  a  40°C  surface.  The  behavioral  end  point 
used  is  the  maximum  elevation  of  the  anterior  portion  of  the  fully  ex- 
tended foot. 


MAGNETIC  FIELDS  AND  OPIOID  SYSTEMS 


303 


live  value  (Amit  and  Galina,  1986).  In  vertebrates,  the 
tonic  activity  of  endogenous  opioid  systems  can  be  in- 
creased by  a  range  of  environmental  stimuli.  In  laboratory 
rodents,  this  "stress" — or  environmentally  induced  an- 
algesia (Amit  and  Galina,  1984) — can  be  recorded  as  an 
increased  latency  of  a  foot-lift  or  tail-flick  response.  Ad- 
ministration of  either  endogenous  opioid  peptides,  such 
as  enkephalin  or  exogenous  opiate  antagonists,  such  as 
the  prototypic  n  opiate  agonist,  morphine,  produces  sim- 
ilar analgesic  effects.  Prototypic  exogenous  opiate  antag- 
onists, such  as  naloxone  or  naltrexone,  can  reverse  or  at- 
tenuate these  analgesic  effects,  and,  in  certain  cases,  can 
reduce  nociceptive  responses  and  induce  hyperanalgesia 
(Martin,  1984). 

Similar  evidence  for  opioid  involvement  in  the  media- 
tion of  antinociception  or  analgesia  and  nociception  is 
present  for  mollusks.  Morphine,  as  well  as  the  endogenous 
opioids  /i-endorphin  and  methionine-enkephalin.  en- 
hance, in  a  dose-dependent  manner,  the  latency  of  the 
nociceptive  responses  ofCepaea  and  the  slug.  Arion.  to  a 
warmed  surface  ( Dalton  and  Widdowson,  1989;  Kavaliers 
ct  ul.,  1983,  1985).  As  in  mammals,  maximum  antino- 
ciceptive  effects  of  morphine  in  Cepaea  are  seen  15-30 
min  after  injection,  with  a  decline  to  basal  thermal  re- 
sponse latencies  by  60-120  min  (Kavaliers  ct  ai.  1983). 
These  antinociceptive  effects  occur  without  any  evident 
effects  on  the  spontaneous  locomotor  activity  or  motor 
abilities  of  the  animals.  The  antinociceptive  effect  of  mor- 
phine is  also  produced  by  the  benzomorphan  levorphanol. 
but  not  by  the  stereoisomer  dextrophan,  suggesting  that 
the  receptor  that  interacts  with  these  opiates  has  stereo- 
specific  requirements  (Hirst  and  Kavaliers.  1987).  Nal- 
oxone suppresses,  and  dose-dependently  reverses,  the  an- 
algesic effects  of  morphine  in  Cepaea,  and  reduces  the 
response  times  (hyperalgesia)  of  particular  morphological 
types  of  Cepaea  that  display  elevated  nociceptive  re- 
sponses (Kavaliers  et  al.,  1983;  Kavaliers,  1989b).  This 
further  supports  opioid  involvement  in  the  mediation  of 
antinociception  and  nociception  in  Cepaea. 

The  specific  n  and  5  opioid  agonists,  (D-Ala:-Me-Phe5, 
Gly-ol)-enkephalin  (DAMGO)  and  (D-Ala2,  D-Leu4)  en- 
kephalin (DADLE),  respectively,  also  have  significant  an- 
tinociceptive effects  in  Cepaea  and  Arion,  suggesting  the 
presence  of  M  and  b  opioid  receptors  (Dalton  and  Wid- 
dowson, 1989;  Kavaliers  et  ai,  1985).  In  addition,  the 
specific  K  opiate  agonist  U-50.488H,  has  significant  an- 
tinociceptive effects  in  Cepaea  (Kavaliers  and  Ossenkopp. 
1989).  As  in  mammals,  the  duration  of  effect  of  U- 
50.488H  is  longer  than  that  of  morphine,  and  there  is  a 
low  sensitivity  to  reversal  by  naloxone.  Taken  together 
with  the  demonstrations  of  K  opioid  binding  sites  and  the 
immunocytochemical  localization  of  the  endogenous  K 
ligand  dynorphin,  in  invertebrates  (Ford  el  al..  1986),  these 


antinociceptive  effects  raise  the  possibility  of  a  K  opioid- 
mediated  antinociceptive  system  in  Cepaea. 

Day-night  rhythms  of  nociception.  Significant  day-night 
rhythms  are  exhibited  in  the  nociceptive  responses  and 
analgesic  effects  of  morphine  in  Cepaea.  These  noctumally 
and  crepuscularly  active  snails  display  elevated  night-time 
levels  of  nociception  and  morphine-induced  analgesia 
under  both  field  and  laboratory  conditions  (Kavaliers  et 
al..  1990).  The  elevated  nocturnal  response  latencies  to  a 
thermal  stimulus  are  reduced  by  naloxone  and  the  diel 
rhythm  of  nociception  can  be  disrupted  by  pretreatment 
with  the  irreversible  /u  opioid  receptor  alkylating  agent, 
fJ-funaltrexamine  (/3-FNA)  (Kavaliers  and  Ossenkopp, 
1991).  This  suggests  that  endogenous  n  opioid  systems 
may  be  involved  in  the  generation  or  expression  of  the 
day-night  rhythm  of  this  measure  of  nociception  in 
Cepaea. 

Stress-induced  analgesia.  In  rodents,  diverse  stimuli 
have  been  shown  to  increase  endogenous  opioid  activity 
and  induce  integrated  adaptive  behavioral  responses,  in- 
cluding analgesia  (Amit  and  Galina,  1986).  Similar  en- 
vironmentally induced  opioid  activation  and  analgesia  is 
also  evident  in  mollusks  and  other  invertebrates  (Kava- 
liers, 1987;  Maldonado  and  Miralto,  1987;  Dalton  and 
Widdowson,  1989;  Valeggia  et  al.,  1989).  Exposure  to  ei- 
ther heat,  centrifugal  rotation,  or  novel  chemical  stimuli 
has  been  shown  to  increase  thermal  nociceptive  thresholds 
of  Cepaea  (Kavaliers,  1987,  1989a).  The  warm-stress-in- 
duced analgesia  is  blocked  by  naloxone  and  the  6  opioid 
antagonist  ICI  154,129,  and  is  suppressed  by  a  24-h  pre- 
treatment with  0-FNA  (Kavaliers,  1987).  Brief  body  (tail) 
pinch  stress  of  the  slugs  Arion  and  Umax  also  resulted  in 
significant  increases  in  their  response  latencies  (Kavaliers 
and  Hirst,  1986;  Dalton  and  Widdowson.  1988).  The  an- 
algesic response  ofLimax  was  blocked  by  naloxone,  while 
that  of  Arion  was  reduced  in  a  dose-dependent  manner 
by  naltrexone  and  the  d  opiate  antagonist  ICI  148,164. 
Moreover,  the  duration  of  the  stress-induced  analgesia  in 
Arion  could  be  prolonged  by  the  injection  of  enkephalin- 
ase  inhibitors  (Dalton  and  Widdowson,  1988).  This  fur- 
ther supports  the  involvement  of  endogenous  opioid  pep- 
tides  in  the  mediation  of  a  number  of  forms  of  stress- 
induced  analgesia  in  gastropod  mollusks.  It  should  be 
noted,  however,  that  although  these  antinociceptive  re- 
sponses are  opioid-mediated,  it  would  be  desirable  to 
demonstrate  cross-tolerance  to  exogenous  opiate-induced 
analgesia,  as  well  as  to  show  changes  in  endogenous  opioid 
peptide  levels  and  receptor  binding. 

Mechanisms.  At  a  biochemical  and  cellular  level,  there 
is  evidence  to  suggest  that  the  antinociceptive  effects  of 
opiates,  in  both  Cepaea  and  rodents,  are  associated  with 
alterations  in  calcium  channel  activity.  Calcium  channels 
are  reported  to  be  involved  in  the  regulation  of  neuronal 
functions  in  mollusks  in  a  manner  similar,  but  not  nee- 


304 


M.  KAVALIERS  AND  K..-P.  OSSENKOPP 


essarily  identical,  to  that  in  vertebrates  (Akaike  el  a/..  1 98 1 ; 
Gerschenfeld  et  a/..  1986;  Hammond  el  at,  1987;  Miller. 
1987).  In  vertebrates,  the  activation  of  n  or  o  opioid  re- 
ceptor types  increases  potassium  channel  conductance  and 
indirectly  reduces  calcium  channel  conductance,  while 
activation  of  K  receptors  causes  a  direct  reduction  in  volt- 
age dependent  calcium  conductance  (North,  1986).  In 
both  cases,  the  net  result  is  a  reduction  in  neuronal  dis- 
charge frequency  and  the  amount  of  transmitter  released. 
In  both  rodents  and  Cepaea,  the  dihydropyridine  (DHP) 
and  non-DHP  calcium  channel  antagonists  diltiazem. 
verapamil,  and  nifedipine  can  reduce  exogenous  opiate 
and  stress-induced  opioid  analgesia  (Kavaliers  and  Os- 
senkopp,  1987,  1989).  This  suggests  similar  roles  for  cal- 
cium-channel-related mechanisms  in  the  mediation  of 
opiate-induced  analgesia  in  mammals  and  mollusks.  In 
addition,  pharmacological  reductions  of  G  protein  activity 
by  pertussis  toxin  pretreatment  have  similar  inhibitory 
effects  on  morphine-induced  analgesia  in  Cepaea  and  ro- 
dents ( Yu  and  Kavaliers,  1991).  This  suggests  that  similar 
intermediary  messenger  systems  are  involved  in  the  me- 
diation of  opiate  effects  in  Cepaea  and  rodents.  Moreover, 
data  also  indicate  that  opiates  have  similar  inhibitory  ef- 
fects on  dopamine  and  possibly  other  monoamine  systems 
in  rodents  and  mollusks  (Stefano,  1982).  These  observa- 
tions suggest  similar  modes  of  action  and  sensitivities  of 
opioid  systems  in  vertebrates  and  mollusks. 

Magnetic  Fields  and  Opioid  Systems 

General  aspects- 
Research  on  the  roles  of  geomagnetic  information  in 
avian  and  invertebrate  orientation  and  migration  has 
provided  some  of  the  most  convincing  results  on  the  bio- 
logical effects  of  magnetic  fields  (Ossenkopp  and  Barbeito, 
1978;  Gould,  1984;  Wiltschko  and  Wiltschko,  1990).  A 
variety  of  other  biological  effects  produced  by  exposure 
to  magnetic  fields  have  also  been  documented  in  both 
invertebrates  and  vertebrates  (reviews  in  Adey,  1981; 
Gould,  1984;  Ossenkopp  and  Kavaliers,  1988).  In  mol- 
lusks, these  effects  of  magnetic  fields  include  alterations 
in  neuronal  activity  and  orientation  behaviors  ( Brown  and 
Webb,  1960;  Brown  et  at,  1960  a,b;  Brown,  1971;  Loh- 
mann  and  Willows,  1987;  Azanza,  1989;  Balaban  et  at, 
1990). 

As  previously  indicated,  among  the  more  dramatic  ac- 
tions of  magnetic  stimuli  in  mammals  are  reversible 
modifications  in  the  effects  of  exogenous  opiates  and  en- 
dogenous opioids.  Natural  geomagnetic  disturbances 
arising  from  intense  solar  activity,  earth  strength,  0.5-1.5 
gauss  60  Hz  magnetic  fields,  relatively  weak  rotating  mag- 
netic fields,  and  stronger  magnetic  fields  associated  with 
diagnostic  magnetic  resonance  imaging  have  all  been 
shown  to  reduce  the  analgesic  effects  of  morphine  in  mice 


(Kavaliers  and  Ossenkopp.  1984.  1986;  Miller  et  at,  1985; 
Ossenkopp  et  at.  1983;  Prato  et  at.  1987). 

Results  of  recent  investigations  with  the  snail  Cepaea, 
have  extended  these  inhibitory  effects  of  magnetic  stimuli 
on  opioid  systems  to  mollusks  ( Kavaliers  and  Ossenkopp, 
1989).  These,  and  additional  findings  from  in  vitro  prep- 
arations (Golding  et  at.  1985).  avian  orientation  (Papi 
and  Luschi,  1991),  and  spatial  learning  in  rodents  (Ka- 
valiers et  at.  199  la),  which  relate  the  effects  of  magnetic 
fields  to  alterations  in  opioid  activity,  suggest  that  a  broad 
range  of  fundamental  opioid-mediated  functions  may  be 
sensitive  to  magnetic  stimuli. 

Magnetic  fields  and  opioid-mediated  nociception  in 
Cepaea 

Rotating  magnetic  fields.  Results  of  investigations  of 
the  effects  of  exposure  to  a  0.5  Hz  rotating  magnetic  field 
(RMF)  on  morphine-induced  antinociception  in  Cepaea 
provided  the  first  direct  evidence  that  magnetic  stimuli 
could  affect  opioid  systems  in  an  invertebrate  (Kavaliers 
and  Ossenkopp,  1989).  As  in  rodents  (Kavaliers  and  Os- 
senkopp, 1986,  1987),  exposure  for  15-30  min  to  a  het- 
erogenous  time-varying  magnetic  field  (0.15-9.0  mT  or 
1.5-90  gauss,  produced  by  two  rotating  horseshoe  mag- 
nets) of  about  0.5  Hz  significantly  reduced  day-time  mor- 
phine-induced analgesia  in  Cepaea  without  any  evident 
effects  on  the  basal  nociceptive  responses  of  saline-treated 
control  animals.  The  rotating  magnetic  fields  also  atten- 
uated the  analgesic  effects  of  the  K  opiate  agonist  U- 
50,488H.  In  addition,  and  as  in  rodents,  exposure  to  the 
rotating  magnetic  fields  reduced  stress-induced  opioid 
analgesia  in  Cepaea.  These  findings  show  that  time-vary- 
ing magnetic  fields  can  significantly  alter  both  exogenous 
opiate-  (n  and  * )  and  endogenous  opioid-induced  analgesia 
in  an  invertebrate.  These  observations  also  raise  the  pos- 
sibility that  exposure  to  magnetic  stimuli  may  compro- 
mise the  expression  of  adaptive  opioid-mediated  behav- 
ioral and  physiological  responses  to  environmental 
stresses.  It  should  be  noted  that  in  control  sham  exposure 
conditions,  where  dummy  weights  rather  than  horseshoe 
magnets  were  used,  there  were  no  effects  on  opioid-me- 
diated antinociception.  In  these  studies  there  was  an 
equivalent  electric  field  in  the  sham  and  magnetic  field 
exposure  conditions.  This  minimizes  the  potential  in- 
volvement of  electric  fields  in  the  inhibition  of  opioid 
analgesia. 

60  Hz  magnetic  fields.  Increasing  concerns  about  the 
possible  health  effects  due  to  exposure  to  high-voltage 
transmission  lines  and  electrical  appliances  in  the  home 
have  been  expressed  (Ahlboom,  1988).  There  have  been 
numerous  reports  documenting  biological  effects  in  ver- 
tebrates following  exposure  to  50  or  60  Hz  magnetic  fields, 
although  relatively  little  is  known  about  the  possible  effects 


MAGNETIC  FIELDS  AND  OPIOID  SYSTEMS 


305 


in  invertebrates.  The  effects  in  vertebrates  have  included 
retardation  in  embryological  development,  changes  in 
behavioral  activity  levels  and  inhibition  of  chemically  and 
electrically  kindled  seizures.  These  effects  are  compatible 
with  alterations  in  the  functioning  of  endogenous  opioid 
systems  (review  in  Ossenkopp  and  Kavaliers,  1988).  This 
speculation  of  opioid  involvement  is  encouraged  by  the 
observation  that  acute  (30-min)  exposure  to  low  intensity 
60  Hz  magnetic  fields  markedly  reduces  morphine-in- 
duced analgesia  levels  in  mice,  with  a  functional  relation- 
ship between  magnetic  field  intensity  and  the  degree  of 
inhibition  of  analgesia  (Ossenkopp  and  Kavaliers,  1987). 

Recently,  it  was  observed  that  exposure  of  C'cpaca  to 
low  intensity  ( 1 .0  gauss,  rms)  60  Hz  magnetic  fields  in  a 
Helmholtz  coil  apparatus,  as  shown  in  Figure  2,  also  re- 
sulted in  an  attenuation  of  morphine-induced  analgesia 
(Kavaliers  et  til..  1990).  Various  durations  of  exposure 
(0.50,  2,  12,  48,  or  120  h)  to  the  60  Hz  fields  reduced  the 
levels  of  morphine-induced  analgesia  in  both  the  light  and 
dark  periods  of  a  12  h  light:  12  h  dark  cycle,  with  the 
magnetic  stimuli  having  significantly  greater  inhibitory 
effects  in  the  dark  period.  The  inhibitory  effects  of  the 
magnetic  fields  were  reversible.  Twenty-four  hours  after 
exposure,  the  levels  of  morphine-induced  analgesia  were 
not  significantly  different  from  pre-exposure  levels  (Kav- 
aliers et  al.,  1990).  These  effects  in  C'cpaca  are  consistent 
with  the  day-night  rhythms  in  the  inhibitory  effects  of 
naloxone  and  60  Hz  and  rotating  magnetic  fields  on  mor- 
phine-induced analgesia  in  nocturnal  rodents  (Kavaliers 
and  Ossenkopp,  1984;  Ossenkopp  and  Kavaliers,  1987). 
The  weak  60  Hz  magnetic  fields  also  significantly  reduced 
the  levels  of  the  elevated  naloxone-sensitive  dark  period 
nociceptive  response  latencies  in  Cepaea,  while  not  af- 
fecting the  lower  level  light  period  responses.  Moreover, 
the  degree  of  attenuation  of  the  analgesic  and  nociceptive 
response  latencies  was  related  to  the  duration  of  exposure 
to  the  60  Hz  magnetic  fields. 

Determinations  were  also  made  of  the  effects  of  60  Hz 
magnetic  fields  on  opioid-mediated  responses  outside  the 
laboratory  under  natural  conditions.  Exposure  to  1 .0  gauss 
60  Hz  magnetic  fields  under  field  conditions  significantly 
attenuated  morphine-induced  antinociception  and  noci- 
ceptive responses  of  Cepaea,  with  the  degree  of  attenuation 
being  related  to  the  duration  of  exposure  to  the  magnetic 
fields  ( Tysdale  e I  al ,  1 99 1 ).  The  60  Hz  fields  also  disrupted 
the  day-night  rhythm  of  nociception,  with  particularly 
marked  alterations  in  responses  occurring  during  the  rap- 
idly changing  light  levels  of  the  twilight  periods  as  shown 
in  Figure  3.  These  field  observations  suggest  a  possible 
relation  between  light  reception  or  changes  and  magnetic 
field  reception  in  Cepaea.  A  connection  between  magnetic 
field  and  light  reception  has  been  previously  theoretically 
postulated  (Leask,  1977)  and  experimentally  indicated  in 
several  species  of  arthropods  and  vertebrates  (Leucht, 


Figure  2.  Helmholtz  coil  apparatus  used  for  generation  of  60  Hz 
magnetic  fields  to  which  the  Ccpuea  nemoralis  were  exposed.  Snails  were 
held  individually  in  translucent  polypropylene  50  ml  centrifuge  tubes 
(10  X  2.5)  containing  a  saturated  atmosphere  and  natural  vegetation. 
The  tubes  were  placed  upright  on  a  platform  (exposure  volume)  in  the 
Helmholtz  coil  apparatus.  The  Helmholtz  coils  consisted  of  100  turns 
of  no.  24  motor  enamel  wire  with  a  resistance  of  about  25  Q  per  coil. 
The  coils  were  100  cm  in  diameter  spaced  103  cm  apart  on  the  Z-axis 
and  attached  to  the  outside  of  a  plywood  frame.  The  coils  were  covered 
with  a  resin  coating  which  immobilized  the  wires  in  the  coils  and  pre- 
vented them  from  vibrating  when  they  were  carrying  current.  Line  current 
(60  Hz)  from  standard  outlets  was  applied  to  the  coils  and  regulated 
with  two  variable  autotransformers.  The  experimental  exposure  volume 
(30  x  30  x  330  cm)  in  which  the  snails  in  the  tubes  were  placed  was 
centered  between  the  energized  coils  on  the  Z-axis.  By  altering  the  voltage 
input  to  the  two  coils,  60  Hz  fields  with  linear  polarity  and  field  intensities 
up  to  1.5  gauss  (rms)  could  be  generated  [a  field  intensity  of  1.0  gauss 
(rms)  was  used  in  the  studies  described  in  the  text].  A  sham  field  exposure 
condition  was  produced  by  turning  of)  the  current  to  the  coils  and  placing 
test  animals  in  the  same  exposure  volume. 


1984,  1990;  Olcese  et  al,  1985.  1988;  Phillips,  1987). 
However,  it  has  not  been  established  whether  light  itself 
is  a  prerequisite  for  reception  of  the  magnetic  field. 

These  observations  with  Cepaea  show  that  exposure  to 
weak  60  Hz  magnetic  fields  significantly  affects  the  diel 
rhythms  of  opioid-mediated  responses  in  both  the  labo- 
ratory and  under  natural  environmental  conditions.  They 
also  show  that  the  degree  of  inhibition  of  opioid-mediated 
responses  is  affected  by  both  the  duration  and  timing  (day- 
night  variations)  of  exposure  to  60  Hz  magnetic  fields. 
These  latter  findings  are  particularly  significant  in  view 
of  the  growing  reports  of  associations  between  prolonged 
exposures  to  low  intensity  50  and  60  Hz  magnetic  fields 
and  the  occurrence  of  various  types  of  neoplasms  (e.g., 
Savitz  et  al..  1988)  and  the  evidence  that  opioid  systems 
can  modulate  tumorigenesis  (Zagon  and  McLaughlin, 
1987).  In  this  regard,  it  is  of  interest  that  the  snails  exposed 
to  the  60  Hz  magnetic  fields  showed,  over  a  two-week 
period  following  exposures,  increased  levels  of  mortality 
relative  to  control  sham-field  exposed  animals,  and  that 
night-time  exposures  resulted  in  greater  mortality  levels 
than  day-time  exposures  (Ossenkopp  et  al.,  1990).  This 


306 


M.   (CAVALIERS  AND  K..-P.  OSSENKOPP 


1830 


2200 


Time   [hr] 

Figure  3.  Examples  of  daytime,  nighttime  and  twilight  period  thermal  (40°C)  response  latencies  (no- 
ciceptive  responses)  of  Cepaea  nemoralis  held  under  natural  summer  (August)  outdoor  (Environmental 
Sciences  Center.  London.  Ontario.  43°  4'  30"  and  8P  18'  30"  W)  light  conditions  and  exposed  to  either  a 
60  Hz  magnetic  field  (1.0  gauss,  rms.  as  described  in  Fig.  2)  or  a  sham  magnetic  field  (sham).  Each  point 
shown  represents  the  mean  nociceptive  response  of  18-24  snails.  Different  groups  of  snails  were  tested  on 
each  of  the  four  days  shown.  For  ease  ot  presentation  standard  errors  are  excluded. 

Exposure  to  the  60  Hz  magnetic  fields  had  no  significant  effects  on  the  daytime  (pre-sunset)  nociceptive 
responses,  but  significantly  (P  <  0.01.  repeated  measures  analysis  of  variance  for  2200  h)  reduced  the  night- 
time response  latencies  as  compared  to  the  sham  exposed  snails  and  other  control  animals  (not  shown). 
Exposure  to  the  magnetic  fields  also  significantly  (P  <  0.05,  for  2000  and  2100  h)  attenuated  the  marked 
increases  in  thermal  response  latencies  that  occurred  during  the  decreasing  light  levels  of  the  twilight  periods 
[civil  (c),  nautical  (n)  and  astronomical  (a)  twilights;  defined  by  the  sun  at  -6°,  -12°  and  -18°,  respectively, 
from  the  horizon].  The  greatest  effects  of  the  60  Hz  magnetic  field  on  nociceptive  responses  occurred  during 
the  nautical  and  astronomical  portions  of  the  twilight  period. 

The  temperatures  and  light  intensities  that  the  snails  were  exposed  to  ranged  from  22  to  28°C  and  100 
to  200  /jw/cm2  (20-40  Mw/cm2  in  the  tubes)  in  the  daytime,  and  from  14  to  22°C  and  0.01  to  10  Mw/cm2 
(0.00 1  - 1 .0  ^w/cnr  in  the  tubes)  in  the  twilight  transitions  and  nighttime.  These  light  (tubes)  and  temperature 
values  were  similar  to  the  conditions  present  in  the  natural  habitat  of  the  snails  (Kavaliers,  1989b).  The 
background  geomagnetic  field  had  a  daytime  horizontal  (H)  intensity  of  0.48  gauss,  a  vertical  (Z)  intensity 
of  0.24  gauss,  and  inclination  (I)  of  75.  The  Helmholtz  coils  were  oriented  with  the  x-axis  oriented  almost 
directly  towards  magnetic  north. 


is  of  relevance  in  view  of  the  suggestions  of  synergistic 
effects  between  exposure  to  magnetic  fields  and  environ- 
mental pollutants  in  the  induction  of  neoplasms  (Adey, 
1987,  1990). 

Mechanisms  of  action  of  magnetic  stimuli  on  opioid 
systems 

The  inhibitory  effects  of  the  magnetic  stimuli  observed 
in  both  the  day-  and  night-time  may  arise  from  the  in- 
creased levels  of  the  magnetic  field  as  compared  to  earth 
strength  fields  or  fluctuations  in  field  strength.  Although 
data  has  been  presented  to  suggest  that  both  of  these  com- 
ponents can  influence  biological  systems  (Adey,  1981; 
Cremer-Bartels  et  at,  1984),  evidence  is  accumulating  that 
the  biological  effects  of  magnetic  fields  are  primarily  due 
to  fluctuations  in  field  strength  (Blackman  et  ai,  1985, 
1989;  Prato  et  at..  1987).  Furthermore,  data  indicate  that 


the  extent  of  the  biological  effects  of  weak  magnetic  fields 
are  dependent  on  the  relative  intensity  and  orientations 
of  both  the  steady  state  [local  geomagnetic  field,  which 
varies  on  a  day-night  basis  (Cremer-Bartels  et  a/..  1984) 
and  oscillating  field  (Blackman  et  ai,  1985;  Prato  et  at, 
1987)].  However,  it  should  be  noted  that  many  behavioral 
and  physiological  responses  show  no  evidence  of  sensitiv- 
ity to  fluctuating  magnetic  fields  (Ossenkopp  and  Kava- 
liers. 1988). 

Magnetic  fields  have  been  proposed  to  alter  the  prop- 
erties and  stability  of  biological  membranes,  their  trans- 
port characteristics,  and  the  intra-  and  extra-cellular  dis- 
tributions and  flux  of  calcium  ions  (Bawin  and  Adey, 
1976;  Adey,  1981,  1989;  Liboflf  et  at.  1 987;  Carson  et  at. 
1990).  Blackman  et  at  (1985,  1989)  indicated  that  ex- 
posure to  various  combinations  of  time-varying  and  local 
geomagnetic  fields  caused  significant  changes  in  the  efflux 
of  calcium  ions  from  in  vitro  preparations  of  chick  brain 


MAGNETIC  FIELDS  AND  OPIOID  SYSTEMS 


307 


tissue.  They  speculated  that  this  effect  of  magnetic  fields 
on  calcium  ion  efflux  might  involve  a  general  property  of 
biological  tissue. 

There  is  evidence  that  the  inhibitory  effects  of  the  mag- 
netic fields  on  opioid  analgesia  also  involve  changes  in 
the  levels,  flux,  and  distribution  of  calcium  ions,  altera- 
tions in  the  functioning  of  calcium  channels,  along  with 
modifications  in  the  coupling  between  opioid  receptors 
and  calcium  channels.  This  is  supported  by  the  findings 
that  the  DHP  and  non-DHP  calcium  channel  antagonists 
diltiazem,  nifedipine,  and  verapamil  significantly  reduce, 
while  the  DHP  calcium  channel  agonist  BAY  K.8644,  sig- 
nificantly enhances  the  inhibitory  effects  of  rotating  mag- 
netic fields  on  morphine-induced  analgesia  in  Cepaea  and 
mice  (Kavaliers  and  Ossenkopp,  1987,  1989).  In  addition, 
the  inhibitory  effects  of  rotating  magnetic  fields  on  murine 
morphine-induced  analgesia  are  reduced  by  the  calcium 
chelator  EGTA,  and  potentiated  by  the  ionophore  A2 1 387 
(Kavaliers  and  Ossenkopp,  1986). 

Magnetic  stimuli  could  affect  calcium  channel  activa- 
tion and  conductance  either  directly  or  indirectly  through 
alterations  of  intermediary  effector  or  messenger  systems. 
The  second  messenger  system  most  commonly  associated 
with  opioid  receptors  and  changes  in  ion  transport  in- 
volves inhibition  of  adenyl  cyclase  through  G  proteins 
(North,  1 986;  Stryer  and  Bourne,  1986).  Administrations 
of  pertussis  toxin,  which  deactivates  G  proteins,  reduce 
opiate-induced  analgesia  in  both  rodents  and  Cepaea 
(Parenti  i>t  al..  1986;  Przewlocki  et  ai,  1987;  Yu  and  Ka- 
valiers, 1990).  Whether  magnetic  fields  affect  G  protein 
activity  is  not  known. 

Calcium-activated,  phospholipid-dependent  protein 
kinase  (protein  kinase  C;  PKC)  also  plays  an  important 
role  in  relaying  transmembrane  signalling  in  diverse  cal- 
cium-dependent cellular  processes  (Kaczmarek,  1987). 
Results  of  studies  with  PKC  activators  and  inhibitors  have 
shown  that  modulation  of  ion  channel  activity  is  an  im- 
portant function  of  PKC  (DeRiemer  et  ai,  1985;  Kacz- 
marek, 1987;  Strong  et  al.,  1987;  Conn  et  al.,  1989).  Rel- 
atively little  is  known  about  the  relations  between  PKC 
and  opioid  receptor  activity,  although  results  of  a  recent 
study  indicate  that  stimulation  of  PKC  with  phorbol  esters 
attenuates  opioid  activity  through  a  decrease  in  G  protein 
activity  (Louie  et  al..  1990). 

There  is,  however,  accumulating  evidence  linking 
magnetic  fields  and  PKC  activity.  Magnetic  stimuli  have 
been  reported  to  augment  the  effects  of  phorbol  esters 
(PKC  activators)  and  increase  PKC  activity  in  a  number 
of  cell  culture  preparations  (Byus  et  al.,  1987;  Adey,  1987, 
1990).  In  Cepaea,  the  isoqinoline  sulfonamides  H-7  and 
H-9,  which  are  specific  inhibitors  of  PKC,  reduce  the  in- 
hibitory effects  of  60  Hz  magnetic  fields  on  morphine- 
induced  analgesia,  whereas  administration  of  the  PKC 
activator  SC-9  augments  the  effects  of  the  magnetic  fields 


(Kavaliers  et  ai,  1991b).  This  suggests  that  the  inhibitory 
effects  of  magnetic  fields  on  opiate-induced  analgesia  in 
Cepaea  may  include  increases  in  PKC  activity.  Whether 
this  involves  effects  on  G  proteins  remains  to  be  deter- 
mined. 

These  mechanisms  of  action  encompass  a  broader  range 
of  effects  than  just  that  of  the  opioid  systems.  However, 
in  view  of  the  broad  range  and  phylogentic  conservation 
of  fundamental  processes  in  which  opioid  systems  are  in- 
volved, these  findings  suggest  that  some  of  the  biological 
effects  of  magnetic  fields  may  arise  through  alterations  of 
opioid  activity. 

Acknowledgments 

We  thank  Susan  Lipa  and  Donna  Tysdale  for  their 
technical  assistance  and  twilight  determinations.  The  re- 
search described  here  was  supported  by  Natural  Science 
and  Engineering  and  Research  Council  of  Canada  grants 
to  M.K.  and  K.P.O. 

Literature  Cited 

Adey,  \V.  R.  1981.  Tissue  interactions  with  non-ionizing  electromag- 
netic fields.  Physiol  Rev  61:  435-513. 

Adey,  W .  R.  1987.  Cell  membranes:  the  electromagnetic  environment 
and  cancer  promotion.  Neurochem.  Res.  13:671-677. 

Adey,  W.  R.  1990.  Joint  actions  of  environmental  nonionizing  elec- 
tromagnetic fields  and  chemical  pollution  in  cancer  promotion.  En- 
viron. Health  Perxpec.  86:  297-305. 

Ahlboom,  A.  1988.  A  review  of  the  epidemiologic  literature  on  magnetic 
fields  and  cancer.  Scan  J  \fork  limiro.  Health  14:  337-343. 

Akaike,  N.,  A.  M.  Brown,  K.  Nishi,  and  V.  Tsuda.  1981.  Actions  of 
verapamil,  diltiazem  and  other  divalent  cations  on  the  calcium  current 
of  Helix  neurons.  Br  J  Phurmacul.  74:  87-95. 

Akil,  H.,  S.  J.  Watson,  M.  E.  I^ewis,  II.  Khachaturian,  and  M.  J.  Walker. 
1984.  Endogenous  opioids:  biology  and  function.  Ann.  Rev.  Neu- 
rosci.  1:  22-256. 

Amil,  Z.,  and  G.  H.  Galina.  1986.  Stress-induced  analgesia:  adaptive 
pain  suppression.  Physiol.  Rev  66:  1091-1 120. 

Azanza,  M.  J.  1989.  Steady  magnetic  fields  mimic  the  effect  of  caffeine 
on  neurons.  Brain  Res.  489:  195-198. 

Balaban,  P.  M.,  N.  I.  Bravarenko,  and  A.  N.  Kuznetzov.  1990.  Influence 
of  a  stationary  magnetic  field  on  bioelectric  properties  of  snail  neurons. 
Bioelectromagnetics  11:  13-25. 

Bawin,  S.  M.,  and  W.  R.  Adey.  1976.  Sensitivity  of  calcium  binding 
in  cerebral  tissue  to  weak  environmental  electric  fields  oscillating  at 
low  frequency.  Proc.  Nat.  Acad.  Set.  USA  73:  1999-2003. 

Besson,  J.-M.,  and  A.  Chaouch.  1987.  Peripheral  and  spinal  mecha- 
nisms of  nociception.  Physiol  Rev  67:  68-186. 

Blackman,  C.  F.,  S.  G.  Beane,  R.  J.  Rabinoowicz,  D.  R.  House,  and 
W.  T.  Joines.  1985.  A  role  for  the  magnetic  field  in  the  radiation- 
induced  efflux  of  calcium  ions  from  brain  tissue  in  vivo.  Bioelec- 
iromagnetics  6:  327-339. 

Blackman,  C.  F.,  L.  S.  Kinney,  D.  E.  House,  and  W.  T.  Joines. 
1989.  Multiple  power-density  windows  and  their  possible  origin. 
Bioelectromagnetics  10:  1 15-128. 

Brown,  F.  A.,  Jr.  1971.  Some  orientational  influences  of  non-visual 
terrestrial  electromagnetic  fields.  Ann.  N.Y.  Acad.  Sci-  188:  224-241. 

Brown,  F.  A.,  Jr.,  and  H.  M.  Webb.  1960.  A  "compass-direction" 
effect  for  snails  in  constant  conditions  and  its  lunar  modulation.  Bio! 
Bull.  119:65-74. 


308 


M.  KAVALIERS  AND  K..-P.  OSSENKOPP 


Brown,  F.  A.,  Jr.,  M.  F.  Bennett,  and  II.  M.  \\  ebb.  1960s.  A  magnetic 
compass  response  of  an  organism.  Biol.  Bull.  119:  65-74. 

Brown,  F.  A.,  Jr.,  VV.  J.  Brett,  M.  F.  Bennett,  and  F.  H.  Barnwell. 
1960b.  Magnetic  response  of  an  organism  and  its  solar  relationships. 
Bio/  Bull.  118:  367-381. 

Byus,  C.  V.,  S.  E.  Pieper.  and  NV.  R.  Adey.  1987.  The  effects  of  low- 
energy  60  Hz  environmental  electromagnetic  fields  upon  the  growth- 
related  enzyme  ornithine  decarboxylase.  Carcinogenesis  8:  1385- 
1389. 

Carson,  J.  J.  L.,  F.  S.  Prato,  D.  J.  Prost,  L.  D.  Diesbourg,  and  S.  J. 
Dixon.  1990.  Time-varying  magnetic  fields  increase  cytosolic  free 
Ca:+  in  HL60  cells.  Am.  J  Physiol  28:  C687-C692. 

Chiles,  C.,  E.  Hawrot,  J.  Gore,  and  R.  Byck.  1989.  Magnetic  field 
modulation  of  receptor  binding.  Magnetic  Resonance  in  Medicine 
10:241-245. 

Conn,  J.  P.,  J.  A.  Strong,  and  L.  K.  Kaczmarek.  1989.  Inhibitors  of 
protein  kinase  C  prevent  enhancement  of  calcium  current  and  action 
potentials  in  peptidergic  neuron  ofAplysia.  J.  Neitrosci.  9:  480-487. 

Cremer-Bartel,    G.,    K.    krause,    G.    Mitoskas,    and    D.    Brodersen. 

1984.  Magnetic  fields  of  the  earth  as  additional  zeitgeber  for  en- 
dogenous rhythms.  Naturwissenschaften  71:  567-574. 

Dalton,  L.  M.,  and  P.  S.  \\iddowson.  1989.  The  involvement  of  opioid 
peptides  in  stress-induced  analgesia  in  the  slug  Arion  ater.  Peplides 
10:9-13. 

De  Riemer,  S.  A.,  J.  A.  Strong,  K.  A.  Albert,  P.  Greengard,  and  L.  K. 
Kaczmarek.  1985.  Enhancement  of  calcium  current  in  Aplysia 
neurons  by  phorbol  ester  and  kinase  C.  Nature  313:  313-316. 

Ford,  R.,  D.  M.  Jackson,  L.  Tetrault,  J.  C.  Torres,  P.  Assanh,  J.  Harper, 
M.  K.  Leung,  and  G.  B.  Stefano.  1986.  A  behavioral  role  for  en- 
kephalins  in  regulating  locomotor  activity  in  the  insect  Leucophaea 
maderae:  evidence  for  high  affinity  kappa-like  opioid  binding  sites. 
Comp.  Biochem.  Physiol.  85C:  61-66. 

Gerschenfeld,  H.  M.,  C.  Hammond,  and  D.  Paupardin-Tritsch.  1986. 
Modulation  of  the  calcium  current  of  molluscan  neurones  by  neu- 
rotransmitters.  J.  Exp.  Biol.  124:  73-91. 

Golding.  G.  P.,  L.  Newboult,  J.  M.  H.  Rees,  and   B.  R.  Varlow. 

1985.  The  effects  of  50  Hz  magnetic  fields  on  opioid  peptide  me- 
diated inhibition  of  guinea  pig  ileum.  Neuropeptides  5:  357-358. 

Gould,  J.  1984.  Magnetic  field  sensitivity  in  animals.  Ann.  Re\'.  Physiol. 
4:  585-598. 

Hammond,  C.,  D.  Paupardin-Tritsch,  A.  C.  Narin,  P.  Greengard,  and 
H.  M.  Gerschenfeld.  1987.  Cholecystokinin  induces  a  decrease  in 
Ca++  current  in  snail  neurons  that  appears  to  be  mediated  by  protein 
kinase  C.  Nature  325:  809-8 1 1 . 

Hirst,  M.,  and  M.  Kavaliers.  1987.  Levorphanol  but  not  dextrophan 
suppresses  the  foot-lifting  response  to  an  aversive  stimulus  in  the 
terrestrial  snail  (Cepaea  nemoralis).  Neuropharmaeology  26:  121- 
123. 

Hollt,  V.  1986.  Opioid  peptide  processing  and  receptor  selectivity.  Ann. 
Rev.  Pharmacol.  Tmicol.  26:  59-77. 

Kaczmarek,  L.  K.  1987.  The  role  of  protein  kinase  C  in  the  regulation 
of  ion  channels  and  neurotransmitter  release.  Trends  Neurosci.  10: 
30-34. 

Kavaliers,  M.  1987.  Evidence  for  opioid  and  non-opioid  forms  of  stress- 
induced  analgesia  in  the  snail.  Cepaea  nemoralis.  Bruin  Rex  410: 
111-115. 

Kavaliers,  M.  1989a.  Evolutionary  and  comparative  aspect  of  noci- 
ception.  Brain  Rex.  Bull.  6:  923-931. 

Kavaliers,  M.  1989b.  Polymorphism  in  opioid  modulation  of  the  ther- 
mal responses  of  the  land  snail  Cepaea  nemoralis  Can.  J.  Zoo/.  67: 
2721-2724. 

Kavaliers,  M.,  and  M.  Hirst.  1986.  Naloxone-reversible  stress-induced 
feeding  and  analgesia  in  the  slug  (Limax  maximus).  Life  Sci.  38: 
203-209. 


Kavaliers,  M.,  M.  Hirst,  and  G.  C.  Teskey.  1983.  A  functional  role 
for  an  opiate  system  in  snail  thermal  behavior.  Science  220:  99-101. 

Kavaliers,  M.,  M.  Hirst,  and  G.  C.  Teskey.  1985.  The  effects  of  opioid 
and  FMRF-amide  peptides  on  thermal  behavior  in  the  snail.  Neu- 
ropharmacology  24:  621-626. 

Kavaliers,  M.,  and  K.-P.  Ossenkopp.  1984.  Exposure  to  rotating  mag- 
netic fields  alters  morphine-induced  behavioural  responses  in  two 
strains  of  mice.  Neuropharmaeology  89:  440-443. 

Kavaliers,  M.,  and  K.-P.  Ossenkopp.  1986.  Magnetic  field  inhibition 
of  morphine-induced  analgesia  and  behavioral  activity  in  mice:  ev- 
idence for  involvement  of  calcium  ions.  Brain  Res  379:  830-838. 

Kavaliers,  M.,  and  K.-P.  Ossenkopp.  1987.  Calcium  channel  involve- 
ment in  magnetic  field  inhibition  of  morphine-induced  analgesia. 
Naunyn-Schmiedebergs  Arch.  Pharmakol.  336:  308-315. 

Kavaliers,  M.,  and  K.-P.  Ossenkopp.  1989.  Magnetic  fields  inhibit 
opioid-mediated  'analgesic'  behaviours  of  the  terrestrial  snail,  Cepaea 
nemoralis.  J.  Comp.  Physio/.  A.  162:  551-558. 

Kavaliers,  M.,  and  K.-P.  Ossenkopp.  1991.  Magnetic  fields,  opioid 
systems  and  day-night  rhythms  of  behavior.  In  Electromagnetic  Fields 
and  Circadian  Rhythms.  \'oi  I.  Advances  in  Circadian  Physiology. 
M.  Moore-Ede  and  S.  Campbell,  eds.  Binkhauser,  Boston,  (in  press). 

Kavaliers,  M.,  K.-P.  Ossenkopp,  and  S.  Lipa.  1990.  Day-night  rhythms 
in  the  inhibitory  effects  of  60  Hz  magnetic  fields  on  opiate-mediated 
'analgesic'  behaviors  of  the  land  snail.  Cepaea  nemoralis.  Brain  Res. 
517:  276-282. 

Kavaliers,  M.,  L.  Eckel,  K.-P.  Ossenkopp,  and  N.  Vu.  1991a.  Enhanced 
learning  of  a  sexually  dimorphic  spatial  task  in  meadow  voles  fol- 
lowing brief  exposure  to  60  Hz  magnetic  fields.  Soc.  Neurosci.  Abstr. 
21  (in  press). 

Kavaliers,  M.,  K.-P.  Ossenkopp,  and  D.  M.  Tysdale.  I991b.  Evidence 
for  the  involvement  of  protein  kinase  C  in  the  modulation  of  mor- 
phine-induced 'analgesia'  and  the  inhibitory  effects  of  exposure  to 
60Hz  magnetic  fields  in  the  land  snail,  Cepaea  nemoralis.  Brain  Res. 
(in  press). 

Kirschvink,  J.  I..,  and  J.  L.Gould.  1981.  Biogenic  magnetite  as  a  basis 
for  magnetic  field  detection  in  animals.  Biosystems  13:  181-201. 

Kream,  R.  M.,  R.  S.  /ukin,  and  G.  B.  Stefano.  1980.  Demonstration 
of  two  classes  of  opiate  binding  sites  in  the  nervous  tissue  of  the 
marine  mollusc,  Mytilim  edulis.  J.  Biol.  Chem.  225:  9218-9224. 

Leask,  M.  J.  M.  1977.  A  physiochemical  mechanism  for  magnetic 
field  detection  by  migratory  birds  and  homing  pigeons.  Nature  267: 
144-145. 

Leucht,  T.  1984.  Responses  to  light  under  varying  magnetic  conditions 
in  the  honey  bee.  Apis  mellificia.  J.  Comp.  Physio/.  A.  154:  865-870. 

Leucht,  T.  1990.  Ineractions  of  light  and  gravity  reception  with  magnetic 
fields  in  \enopit.s  lacvis.  J.  Exp.  Biol.  148:  325-334. 

Leung,  M.  K.,  and  G.  B.  Stefano.  1984.  Isolation  and  identification  of 
enkephalins  in  pedal  ganglia  of  Mytilus  edulis  (Mollusc)  Proc.  Nat. 
Acad.  Sci.  USA  81:  955-958. 

Leung,  M.  K.,  and  G.  B.  Stefano.  1987.  Comparative  neurobiology  of 
opioids  in  invertebrates  with  special  attention  to  senescent  alterations. 
Prog.  Neurobiol.  28:  131-159. 

Leung,  M.  K.,  H.  H.  Boer,  J.  van  Minnen,  J.  Lundy,  and  G.  B  Stefano. 
1990.  Evidence  for  an  enkephalmergic  system  in  the  nervous  system 
of  the  pond  snail,  Lymnaea  slagna/is.  Brain  Res.  531:  66-71. 

Liboff,  A.  R.,  R.  J.  Rozek,  M.  L.  Sherman,  B.  R.  McLeod,  and  S.  D. 
Smith.  1987.  45Ca  cyclotron  resonance  in  human  lymphocytes.  J. 
Bioe/ecl  6:  13-22. 

Liboff,  A.  R.,  and  B.  R.  McLeod.  1988.  Kinetics  of  channelised  mem- 
brane ions  in  magnetic  fields.  Bioelectromagnetics  9:  39-5 1 . 

Liburdy,  R.  P.,  R.  Tenforde,  and  R.  L.  Magin.  1986.  Magnetic-field 
induced  drug  permeability  in  liposome  vesicles.  Radial.  Res.  108: 
102-111. 


MAGNETIC  FIELDS  AND  OPIOID  SYSTEMS 


309 


Lohmann,  K.  J.,  and  A.  O.  D.  Willows.  1987.  Lunar-modulated  geo- 
magnetic orientation  by  a  marine  mollusk.  Science  235:  231-235. 

Louie,  A.  K.,  E.  S.  Bass,  J.  Zahn,  P.  E.  Law,  and  H.  H.  Loh. 
1990.  Attenuation  of  opioid  receptor  activity  by  phorbol  esters  in 
neuroblastoma  X  Glioma  NG  108-15  Hybrid  cells.  J  Pharmacol 
E\p  Therp  253:401-407. 

Maldonado.  H.,  and  A.  Miralto.  1987.  Effects  of  morphine  and  nal- 
oxone  on  a  defensive  response  of  the  mantis  shnmp,  Si/iu/lti  mantis 
J  Cinnp.  Physio/.  A147:  455-459. 

Martin,  \V.  R.  198-4.  Pharmacology  of  opioids.  Pharmncol  Rev  35: 
283-323. 

Miller,  R.  J.  1987.  Multiple  calcium  channels  and  neuronal  function. 
Science  325:  46-52. 

Miller,  D.  B.,  C.  F.  Blackman,  and  J.  S.  Alie.  1985.  Behavioral  responses 
of  morphine-treated  mice  to  ELF  magnetic  fields.  BioelectrrJrnagn. 
Soc.  Meet  Ahstr  1985:  54. 

North,  R.  A.  1986.  Opioid  receptor  types  and  membrane  ion  channels. 
TINS  9:  114-117. 

Olcesc,  J.,  S.  Reuss,  and  L.  Vollrath.  1985.  Evidence  for  the  involve- 
ment of  the  visual  system  in  mediating  magnetic  field  effects  on  pineal 
melatonin  synthesis  in  the  rat.  Brain  Res  333:  382-384. 

Olcese,  J.,  S.  Reuss,  J.  Stehle,  S.  Steinlecher,  and  L.  Vollrath. 
1988.  Response  of  the  mammalian  retina  to  experimental  alteration 
of  the  ambient  magnetic  field.  Brain  Rex.  448:  325-330. 

Ossenkopp,  K.-P.,  and  R.  Barbeito.  1978.  Bird  orientation  and  the 
geomagnetic  field:  a  review.  Neurosd.  Bioheltav.  Rev  2:  255-270. 

Ossenkopp,  K.-P.,  and  M.  Kavaliers.  1987.  Morphine-induced  anal- 
gesia and  exposure  to  60  Hz  magnetic  fields:  inhibition  of  nocturnal 
analgesia  is  a  function  of  magnetic  field  intensity.  Brain  Res  418: 
356-360. 

Ossenkopp,  K.-P.,  and  M.  Kavaliers.  1988.  Clinical  and  applied  aspects 
of  magnetic  field  exposure:  a  possible  role  for  the  endogenous  opioid 
systems.  J.  Bioelect.  7:  189-208. 

Ossenkopp.  K.-P.,  M.  Kavaliers,  and  M.  Hirst.  1983.  Reduced  mor- 
phine analgesia  in  mice  following  a  geomagnetic  disturbance.  Neu- 
rosci.  Lett.  40:  321-325. 

Ossenkopp,  K.-P.,  M.  Kavaliers,  and  S.  M.  Lipa.  1990.  Increased 
mortality  in  land  snails  (Cepaea  nemoralis)  exposed  to  powerline 
(60-Hz)  magnetic  fields  and  effects  of  the  light-dark  cycle.  Neurosd. 
Lett  114:  89-94. 

Papi,  F.,  and  P.  Luschi.  1991.  Pigeon  navigation:  naloxone  injection 
and  magnetic  disturbance  have  a  similar  effect  on  initial  orientation. 
Rend.  Ace.  Na:.  Lincei  (in  press). 

Parent!,  M.,  F.  Tirone,  G.  Giagnoni,  N.  Pecora,  and  D.  Parolaro. 
1986.  Pertussis  toxin  inhibits  the  antinociceptive  action  of  morphine 
in  the  rat.  Eur.  J  Pharmacol.  124:  357-359. 

Phillips,  J.  B.  1987.  Specialized  visual  receptors  respond  to  magnetic 
field  alignment  in  the  bowfly  (Calliphora  vicina).  Soc.  Neurosd.  Ahstr. 
13:  397. 

Prato,  F.  S.,  K.-P.  Ossenkopp,  and  M.  Kavaliers.  1987.  Nuclear  mag- 
netic resonance  inhibition  of  morphine-induced  analgesia  in  mice: 
differential  effects  of  the  static,  radio-frequency  and  time-varying 
magnetic  field  components.  Mag.  Reson.  Imag.  5:  9-14. 

Przewlocki,  R.,  T.  Costa,  J.  Lang,  and  A.  Herz.  1987.  Pertussis  toxin 
abolishes  the  antmociception  mediated  by  opioid  receptors  in  rat 
spinal  cord.  Eur.  J  Pharmacol.  144:  91-93. 

Rodgers,  R.  J.,  and  J.  I.  Randall.  1988.     Environmentally-induced  an- 


algesia: situational  factors,  mechanisms,  and  significance.  Pp  107- 
147  in  Endorphins,  Opiates  ami  Behavioural  Processes.  R.  J.  Rodgers 
and  S.  J.  Cooper,  eds.  Wiley,  Chichester. 

Santoro,  C.,  L.  M.  Hall,  and  R.  S.  Zukin.  1990.  Characterization  of 
two  classes  of  opioid  binding  sites  in  Drosoplnla  melanogasler  head 
membranes./  Neurochem.  54:  154-170. 

Savitz,  D.  A.,  H.  Watchel,  F.  A.  Barnes,  E.  M.  John,  and  J.  G.  Tvrdik. 
1988.  Case-control  study  of  childhood  cancer  and  exposure  to  60- 
Hz  magnetic  fields.  Am  J  Epulemiol  128:  21-38. 

Scharrer,  B.,  G.  B.  Stefano,  and  M.  K.  Leung.  1988.  Opioid  mechanisms 
in  insects,  with  special  attention  to  Leucophaea  maderae.  Cell.  Mol. 
Neurobiol.  8:  269-284. 

Semm,  P.,  T.  Schneider,  and  L.  Vollrath.  1980.  The  effects  of  an  earth- 
strength  magnetic  field  on  the  electrical  activity  of  pineal  cells.  Nature 
288:  607-608. 

Sherrington,  C.  S.  1906.  The  Intcxrutive  Action  of  the  Nervous  System. 
Yale  University  Press,  New  Haven. 

Stefano,  G.  B.  1982.  Comparative  aspects  of  opioid-dopamine  inter- 
action. Cell.  Mol.  Neurohiol  2:  167-182. 

Stefano,  G.  B.  1989.  Role  of  opioid  neuropeptides  in  immunoregu- 
lation.  Pr»t>.  Neurohiol.  33:  149-153. 

Stefano,  G.  B.,  M.  K.  Leung,  X.  /hao,  and  B.  Scharrer.  1989.  Evidence 
for  the  involvement  of  opioid  neuropeptides  in  the  adherence  and 
migration  of  immunocompetent  invertebrate  hemocytes.  Proc.  Nat. 
Acad.  Sci.  USA  86:  626-630. 

Strong,  J.  A.,  A.  P.  Fox,  R.  W.  Tsien,  and  L.  K.  Kaczmarek. 
1987.  Stimulation  of  protein  kinase  C  recruits  covert  calcium 
channels  in  Ap/ysui  hag  cell  neurons.  Nature  325:  714-717. 

Stryer,  L.,  and  H.  R.  Bourne.  1986.  G  proteins:  a  family  of  signal 
transducers.  .  I  nn.  Rev  Cell  liiol  2:391-419. 

Tysdale,  D.,  S.  M.  Lipa,  K.-P.  Ossenkopp,  and  M.  Kavaliers. 
1991.  Inhibitory  effects  of  60Hz  magnetic  fields  on  opiate-induced 
'analgesia'  in  the  land  snail,  Cepaea  nemoralis.  under  natural  con- 
ditions. Physiol  Behiiv  49:  53-56. 

Valeggia,  C.,  E.  Ferandez-Duque,  and  II.  Maldonado.  1989.  Danger 
stimulus-induced  analgesia  in  the  crab  Chasmagnethus  granulatus. 
Brain  Res  481:  304-308. 

Walters.  E.  T.  1987.  Site-specific  sensitization  of  defensive  reflexes  in 
Aplysia:  a  simple  model  of  long-term  hyperalgesia.  J  Neurosd.  1: 
400-407. 

Walters,  E.  T.,  and  M.  T.  Erickson.  1986.  Directional  control  and  the 
functional  organization  of  defensive  responses  in  Aplysia.  J.  Comp. 
Physio/.  A  159:  339-351. 

Wiltschko,  W.,  and  R.  Wiltschko.  1990.  Magnetic  orientation  and 
celestial  cues  in  migratory  orientation.  Experentia  46:  342-352. 

Yu,  N.,  and  M.  Kavaliers.  1991.  Pertussis  toxin  reduces  the  day-night 
rhythm  of  nociception  and  opioid  mediated  antinociception  in  the 
land  snail  Cepaea  nemoralis.  Peptides  (in  press). 

Zagon,  I.  S.,  and  P.  J.  McLaughlin.  1989.  Opioid  antagonist  modu- 
lation of  murine  neuroblastoma:  a  profile  of  cell  proliferation  and 
opioid  peptides  and  receptors.  Brain  Res  480:  16-28. 

Zispcr,  B.,  M.  R.  Ruff,  J.  B.  O'Neill,  C.  C.  Smith,  W.  J.  Higgins,  and 
C.  B.  Pert.  1988.  The  opiate  receptor:  a  single  1 10  kDa  recognition 
molecule  appears  to  be  conserved  in  Tetrahymena  leech  and  rat. 
Brain  Res  463:  296-304. 


Reference:  Biol.  Bull  180:  310-317.  (April,  1991) 


Oxidative  Breakdown  Products  of  Catecholamines  and 

Hydrogen  Peroxide  Induce  Partial  Metamorphosis 

in  the  Nudibranch  Phestilla  sibogae  Bergh 

(Gastropoda:  Opisthobranchia) 

ANTHONY  PIRES  AND  MICHAEL  G.  HADFIELD 

Kewalo  Marine  Laboratory.  P.B.R.C..  University  of  Hawaii,  41  Afwi  St.,  Honolulu.  Hawaii  96813 


Abstract.  Veliger  larvae  of  the  aeolid  nudibranch  Phes- 
tilla sibogae  metamorphose  in  response  to  a  soluble  factor 
from  their  prey  coral,  Porites  compressa.  Metamorphosis 
begins  with  destruction  of  the  velum,  a  ciliated  structure 
used  for  swimming  and  feeding.  Previous  investigation 
had  shown  that  P.  sibogae  larvae  exposed  to  certain  cat- 
echolamines  lost  the  velum,  but  then  failed  to  complete 
any  subsequent  steps  characteristic  of  natural  coral-in- 
duced metamorphosis.  Because  catecholamines  oxidize 
rapidly  in  seawater,  we  have  re-examined  morphogenic 
effects  of  catecholamines  using  superfusion  chambers  that 
allow  periodic  replacement  of  test  solutions.  We  report 
that  fresh,  unoxidized  catecholamines  do  not  induce  velar 
loss,  but  that  this  morphogenic  activity  develops  in  aged, 
oxidized  solutions  of  a  variety  of  catecholamines  and  other 
catechol  compounds.  Evidence  is  presented  that  this  ac- 
tivity is  attributable  to  hydrogen  peroxide,  a  byproduct 
of  catechol  autoxidation.  Hydrogen  peroxide  induces  velar 
loss  at  10~4  M.  The  possible  relationship  of  peroxide-in- 
duced velar  loss  to  natural  coral-induced  metamorphosis 
is  discussed. 

Introduction 

Chemical  and  neural  mechanisms  governing  meta- 
morphosis in  marine  invertebrates  have  long  been  of  in- 
terest both  to  ecologists  seeking  to  understand  recruitment 


Received  14  August  1990:  accepted  6  November  1990. 

Abbreviations:  CI:  natural  coral-derived  metamorphic  inducer;  ASW: 
artificial  seawater.  FSW:  filtered  seawater:  EP:  (-)epmephrine;  NE: 
(-)norepinephnne;  IP:  (-)isoproterenol;  DA:  dopamine;  DOPA:  L-B- 
3,  4-dihydroxyphenylalanine;  DOPAC:  3.4-dihydroxyphenylacetic  acid: 
DOM  A:  3,4-dihydroxymandelic  acid;  DOB:  1,2-dihydroxybenzene; 
HVA:  homovanillic  acid;  OCT:  octopamine. 


of  larvae  into  adult  populations,  and  to  more  reductionist 
biologists  who  view  invertebrate  larvae  as  excellent  model 
systems  for  exploring  the  regulation  of  development 
(Hadneld,  1986).  Larvae  of  the  aeolid  nudibranch  Phes- 
tilla sibogae  metamorphose  upon  exposure  to  a  water- 
soluble  factor  derived  from  the  stony  coral  Porites  com- 
pressa, P.  sibogae's  adult  prey.  Efforts  to  isolate  and  iden- 
tify the  coral-derived  metamorphic  inducer  (CI,  Hadneld 
and  Pennington,  1990)  have  been  accompanied  by  the 
screening  of  a  wide  range  of  chemical  species  for  their 
capacity  to  induce  metamorphosis  (Hadneld,  1984;  Hirata 
and  Hadneld,  1986;  Yool  et  al,  1986:  Pennington  and 
Hadneld.  1989).  Any  such  morphogens  discovered  by  this 
second  approach  can  be  evaluated  as  possible  structural 
analogues  of  CI,  or  as  molecules  involved  in  internal 
transduction  of  the  CI  signal,  or  as  regulators  of  devel- 
opmental mechanisms  that  normally  unfold  as  a  conse- 
quence of  metamorphic  induction  by  CI.  Known  neu- 
rotransmitters  and  neurohormones  are  among  the  plau- 
sible candidates  for  all  three  of  these  roles  (D.  E.  Morse, 
this  symposium;  Bonar  et  al.,  1990).  If  a  morphogenic 
response  can  be  induced  by  application  of  a  known  neu- 
roactive  compound,  certain  plausible  hypotheses  may  be 
made  about  what  sorts  of  receptors  and  internal  trans- 
duction systems  might  mediate  natural  metamorphosis, 
and  appropriate  experiments  designed  to  test  the  impli- 
cated mechanisms. 

Hadneld  (1984)  reported  that  larvae  exposed  to  the  cat- 
echolamines epinephrine  (10~4  A/)  or  norepinephrine 
(10~3  At)  would  often  undergo  a  partial  metamorphosis 
restricted  to  loss  of  the  velum  (a  ciliated  larval  swimming 
and  feeding  organ),  not  followed  by  any  of  the  subsequent 
steps  in  the  morphogenic  sequence  characteristic  of  nat- 


310 


MORPHOGENIC  EFFECT  OF 


311 


ural  metamorphosis  (described  in  Bonar  and  Hadfield, 
1974;  Hadfield.  1978).  A  difficulty  in  the  interpretation 
of  this  result  was  that  catechols  autoxidize  rapidly  to  qui- 
nones  in  alkaline  aqueous  solutions,  in  a  multi-step  re- 
action that  generates  hydrogen  peroxide  (H:O;.).  Figure  1 
shows  the  net  result  of  this  reaction;  its  mechanism,  the 
nature  of  intermediate  products  and  regulation  by  pH 
and  various  catalysts  have  been  explored  for  several  cat- 
echolamines  and  are  the  subject  of  an  extensive  literature 
(Heacock,  1959;  Hawley  et  al..  1967;  Misra  and  Fridovich, 
1972;  Graham,  1978;  Cohen,  1983).  At  25°C,  a  1CT4  M 
solution  ofepinephrine  in  MBL  artificial  seawater  (ASW; 
Cavanaugh.  1956)  Tris-buffered  to  pH  8.2  begins  to  turn 
visibly  pink  within  15  min  due  to  the  appearance  of  the 
quinone  oxidation  intermediate,  adrenochrome.  Maxi- 
mum adrenochrome  concentration,  measured  spectro- 
photometrically,  is  attained  within  3  h  (Piresand  Hadfield, 
unpub.  data).  We  therefore  decided  to  re-examine  mor- 
phogenic  effects  of  catecholamines  and  related  compounds 
using  superfusion  chambers  that  permit  rapid  periodic 
replacement  of  test  solutions  to  control  for  catechol  au- 
toxidation.  Our  goal  was  to  determine  whether  the  pre- 
viously reported  partial  metamorphosis  was  indeed  due 
to  catecholamines,  or  to  some  product  of  catecholamine 
oxidation. 

We  report  that  partial  metamorphosis  (velar  loss)  is 
induced  in  P.  sibogae  by  solutions  of  any  of  several  cat- 
echol compounds  aged  in  ASW  or  by  H:O2  but  not  by 
fresh  catecholamines.  We  also  provide  evidence  that  the 
morphogenic  potency  of  aged  catechols  is  due  to  H2O2  or 
a  derivative  oxygen  species  generated  as  a  consequence 
of  catechol  autoxidation.  These  results  emphasize  the  need 
for  caution  in  interpreting  biological  effects  of  bath-applied 
catecholamines  and  other  unstable  chemical  species.  Our 
results  also  suggest  testable  hypotheses  concerning  possible 
roles  of  H2O2  and  oxygen  radicals  in  natural  coral-induced 
metamorphosis. 


Materials  and  Methods 


Larval  culture 


All  larvae  used  in  these  experiments  were  taken  from 
our  laboratory  culture  system.  Adult  P.  sibogae  were  kept 
together  with  field-collected  heads  of  their  prey  coral  P. 
compressa  in  outdoor  sea  tables  supplied  with  running 
unfiltered  seawater  (~25°C).  Egg  masses  deposited  on 
the  coral  were  collected  daily  and  transferred  to  .22  /im 
filtered  seawater  (FSW).  Eggs  developed  at  25°C  in  aerated 
glass  beakers  in  an  incubator  and  were  mechanically 
hatched  at  day  6  post-fertilization.  Subsequent  culture 
procedures  were  as  previously  described  (Miller  and  Had- 
field, 1 986)  except  that  larval  culture  chambers  continued 
to  be  maintained  in  an  incubator  at  25 °C  after  hatching. 


Figure  1.  Generalized  autoxidation  of  a  catechol  to  a  quinone,  with 
the  production  of  hydrogen  peroxide  (H:O:).  For  catecholamines.  this 
reaction  may  also  involve  cyclization  of  the  side  chain  R. 


Experiments  were  conducted  on  10-day-old  (post-fer- 
tilization), unfed  larvae.  These  larvae  are  facultative 
planktotrophs;  under  our  culture  conditions  nearly  all  10- 
day-old  veligers  are  competent  for  metamorphosis  without 
having  to  feed  (Kempf  and  Hadfield,  1985;  Miller  and 
Hadfield,  1986). 

Preparation  of  test  solutions 

(-)Epinephrine  (EP),  (-)norepinephrine  (NE),  (-)iso- 
proterenol  (IP),  dopamine  (DA),  L-B-3,  4-dihydroxyphe- 
nylalanine  (DOPA),  3,4-dihydroxyphenylacetic  acid 
(DOPAC),  3,4-dihydroxymandelic  acid  (DOMA),  cate- 
chol (1,2-dihydroxybenzene,  DOB),  homovanillic  acid 
(HVA),  octopamine  (OCT).  acetylsalicylic  acid  (aspirin), 
and  thymol-free  bovine  catalase  were  purchased  from 
Sigma  Chemical  Co.  (St.  Louis,  Missouri).  The  first  eight 
compounds  above,  all  of  which  contain  a  catechol  group, 
are  sometimes  referred  to  genetically  in  the  text  as  "cat- 
echol compounds"  or  "catechols."  To  avoid  confusion, 
catechol  itself  is  referred  to  as  1,2-dihydroxybenzene 
(DOB).  All  monoamines  were  obtained  as  hydrochloride 
salts  except  for  EP  (bitartrate)  and  DOPA  (free  acid). 
Pharmaceutical  3%  H2O2  (Parke-Davis)  was  purchased 
locally.  Stock  solutions  of  test  compounds  were  made  fresh 
daily  in  deionized  water  at  10  times  the  desired  final  con- 
centration and  then  diluted  into  l.lx  normal  strength 
MBL  ASW  (Cavanaugh,  1956)  buffered  to  pH  8.1-8.2 
with  10  mMTrizma®  (Sigma).  This  practice  yielded  ASW 
solutions  of  test  compounds  that  were  ionically  equivalent 
to  normal  strength  (l.OX)  MBL  ASW. 

Many  of  the  experiments  presented  in  this  paper  were 
conducted  to  assay  morphogenic  potencies  of  fresh  versus 
aged  (oxidized)  solutions  of  various  catechol  compounds. 
Aged  solutions  of  test  compounds  in  ASW  were  prepared 
as  above  and  allowed  to  stand  10-14  h  at  23-25°C.  Fresh 
solutions  were  prepared  in  the  same  way  from  test  com- 
pound stocks  and  1.1X  MBL  ASW  but  were  mixed  im- 
mediately before  use,  as  were  ASW  solutions  of  H2O2  for 
experiments  on  morphogenic  effects  of  H2O2. 

Experimental  chambers 

Experimental  chambers  (Fig.  2)  were  designed  to  permit 
rapid,  frequent  replacement  of  test  solutions  during  ex- 


312 


A.  PIRES  AND  M.  G.  HADFIELD 


periments.  Each  chamber  is  assembled  from  two  sym- 
metrical halves,  constructed  as  follows.  The  body  of  each 
half-chamber  is  made  from  the  base  of  a  disposable  plastic 
spectrophotometer  cuvette  cut  to  a  volume  of  1  cm1.  An 
18-gauge  hypodermic  needle,  cut  to  a  length  of  1-2  mm, 
is  inserted  through  a  small  hole  in  the  base  of  the  half- 
chamber  and  cemented  in  place  with  silicone  rubber 
aquarium  sealant  (Dow-Corning  or  equivalent).  This  fea- 
ture allows  replacement  of  test  solutions  with  a  syringe. 
Silicone  rubber  gaskets  on  the  open  rim  of  each  half- 
chamber  seal  a  mesh  barrier  (100  jum  Nitex®)  that  sepa- 
rates the  two  halves.  The  halves  are  clamped  together  with 
rubber  bands. 

Our  method  for  making  the  gaskets  is  generally  useful 
for  the  construction  of  small  watertight  apparatuses.  First, 
silicone  rubber  aquarium  sealant  is  applied  to  the  gasket- 
bearing  surface  to  a  slightly  greater  depth  than  the  desired 
thickness  of  the  finished  gasket.  Then  a  microscope  slide 
(or  other  piece  of  smooth  flat  glass)  smeared  with  a  very 
thin  film  of  silicone  stopcock  grease  is  laid  down  on  the 
wet  sealant  and  lightly  pressed  down  until  good  contact 
is  visible  between  sealant  and  glass  along  the  entire  surface 
of  the  gasket.  After  the  sealant  has  cured,  the  glass  can  be 
"popped"  off  the  finished  gasket  with  a  razor  blade. 

Comparisons  of  morphogenic  potencies  of  fresh  and 
aged  test  solutions  (Figs.  4  and  6)  were  carried  out  in  the 
superfusion  chambers  according  to  the  following  protocol. 
Each  chamber  was  loaded  with  17-1 12  larvae  using  a  pi- 
pet.  After  the  chamber  halves  were  assembled,  each 
chamber  was  flushed  with  10  ml  (  =  5X  chamber  volume) 
of  test  solution.  Thereafter,  test  solutions  were  replaced 
in  the  same  manner  every  30  min  until  a  total  exposure 
time  of  7  h  had  elapsed.  Then  the  chambers  were  flushed 
with  FSW  and  larvae  washed  out  into  Slender  dishes. 
Larvae  were  scored  for  velar  loss  16-24  h  later  (criteria 
below).  In  these  experiments  one  set  of  3  or  4  chambers 
was  run  with  fresh  test  solutions  (prepared  as  above  for 
each  solution  change),  while  another  set  was  run  simul- 
taneously with  aged  solutions  (prepared  as  above  the  night 
before  the  experiment).  The  solution  changing  procedure, 
as  well  as  other  aspects  of  physical  manipulation  of  the 
larvae,  was  identical  in  fresh  solution  and  aged  solution 
treatments. 

Experiments  on  dose-dependent  morphogenic  effects 
of  H2O:  and  of  aged  DA  (Figs.  5  and  7)  were  performed 
in  6  cm  Stender  dishes.  Larvae  (53-220  per  dish)  were 
exposed  to  several  concentrations  of  H:O:  in  ASW  for  7 
h  without  solution  changes,  and  then  washed  into  FSW 
for  scoring  as  above. 

Scoring  oj  velar  loss 

Different  degrees  of  velar  loss  are  described  and  illus- 
trated below.  For  scoring  purposes,  larvae  able  to  swim 


OSF 


,SG 


I  SF 


Figure  2.  Superfusion  chamber  used  in  experiments  comparing 
morphogenic  potencies  of  fresh  versus  aged  solutions  of  catechols.  Larvae 
are  retained  in  the  lower  half-chamber  (HC)  by  a  Nitex  mesh  barrier 
(NM).  held  in  place  by  silicone  rubber  gaskets  (SG).  Inlet  and  outlet 
syringe  fittings  (1SF  and  OSF)  allow  periodic  flushing  with  a  syringe 
containing  test  solution.  Total  chamber  volume  is  2  cm1.  Half-chambers 
are  held  together  during  use  by  rubber  bands  (not  shown). 


freely  and  climb  through  the  water  column  were  consid- 
ered to  be  intact.  Such  individuals  never  showed  any  ev- 
idence of  velar  reduction  when  examined  at  SOX  mag- 
nification. Larvae  lying  on  the  bottom  of  the  dish  or 
swimming,  but  failing  to  clear  the  bottom,  were  scored 
as  "velum  lost"  if  any  of  the  large  ciliated  cells  at  the  velar 
margin  were  missing,  or  if  the  velar  lobes  were  noticeably 
shortened.  Instances  of  partial  (Fig.  3B)  and  complete  (Fig. 
3C)  velar  loss  were  combined  and  divided  by  the  total 
number  of  larvae  to  obtain  a  frequency  of  velar  loss  for 
each  trial. 

Results 

Induction  of  velar  loss  by  aged  catechols  and  H:O: 

Loss  of  the  velum,  one  of  the  major  morphological 
transformations  occurring  during  metamorphosis,  can  be 
considered  a  partial  metamorphosis  (Bonar  and  Hadfield, 
1974;  Hadfield,  1984).  We  consistently  found  that  high 
proportions  of  larvae  lost  some  or  all  of  the  velum  (except 
for  small  remnant  clumps  of  cephalic  supportive  cells) 
when  exposed  for  7  h  to  aged  solutions  of  any  of  the  cat- 


MORPHOGENIC  EFFECT  OF  H,O, 

B  C 


313 


Figure  3.  Velar  loss  in  Phestilla  sibogae.  A.  Lateral  view  of  11-day-old,  untreated  larva.  Animal  was 
photographed  with  intact  velum  (arrow)  partly  withdrawn  into  the  larval  shell  to  keep  velum  in  same  plane 
of  focus  as  the  rest  of  the  animal:  foot  (f)  is  partly  extended.  Outlines  of  large  ciliated  cells  are  visible  along 
the  velar  margin.  B.  Partial  velar  loss  in  an  1 1-day-old  larva  treated  with  10~4  Kf  H:O:  on  day  10.  C. 
Complete  velar  loss  in  an  1 1-day-old  larva  treated  with  2  •  10"1  M  H:O;  on  day  10.  Differences  between 
larvae  in  shape  of  foot  represent  a  range  of  movement  independent  of  velar  loss.  Scale  bar  =  100  pm. 


echol  compounds  tested,  or  to  H2O2,  but  not  when  ex- 
posed to  fresh  catecholamines.  Figure  3  illustrates  velar 
loss  in  response  to  H2O2  but  it  could  just  as  well  indicate 
the  results  obtained  with  aged  solutions  of  catechols.  An 
1 1  day-old  untreated  larva  with  intact  velum  is  shown  in 
Figure  3A.  Velar  loss  induced  by  H2O2  or  by  aged  cate- 
chols (or  by  CI)  begins  with  the  detachment  of  the  large 
ciliated  cells  at  the  velar  margin.  These  cells  are  cast  oft" 
intact,  and  their  cilia  continue  to  beat  for  some  time  after 
detachment.  After  separation  of  these  cells  begins,  regres- 
sion of  non-ciliated  supportive  cells  of  the  velar  lobes  be- 
comes apparent.  This  state  of  partial  velar  loss  is  depicted 
in  Figure  3B.  This  state  is  stable  in  that  velar  loss  will 
proceed  no  further  in  larvae  washed  out  of  the  H:O2  or 
aged  catechol  treatment  into  FSW.  If  the  above  treatments 
are  applied  in  sufficiently  high  concentration  (see  below), 
7-h  treatment  results  in  detachment  of  all  the  large  ciliated 
velar  cells  and  detachment  or  regression  of  remaining  tis- 
sues, leaving  small  cephalic  mounds  of  supportive  cells 
where  the  velar  lobes  had  been  (Fig.  3C).  Velar  loss  in 
response  to  these  treatments,  like  that  seen  in  natural 
coral-induced  metamorphosis,  is  highly  tissue-specific. 
Other  ciliated  epithelia  (of  the  foot,  for  example)  remain 
intact.  When  velar  loss  is  induced  with  H2O2  or  aged  cat- 
echols, metamorphosis  does  not  proceed  beyond  this 
point.  However,  if  such  larvae  are  then  exposed  to  CI, 
many  will  right  themselves  on  the  foot,  take  up  the  settled 
posture  characteristic  of  natural  metamorphosis  (Hadneld, 
1978),  and  complete  metamorphosis  in  an  apparently 
normal  fashion. 


Comparison  of  effects  of  fresh  and  aged  catechols 

Morphogenic  effects  of  fresh  and  aged  solutions  of  the 
catecholamines  EP,  NE,  and  IP  (2  X  1(T4  M)  and  DA 
(10~4  M)  were  quantitatively  compared  (Fig.  4).  Dra- 
matically different  results  were  obtained  in  parallel  trials 
using  aged  and  fresh  solutions  of  the  same  compounds, 
replaced  every  30  min  during  the  7-h  exposure  period. 
Most  larvae  lost  some  or  all  of  the  velum  after  exposure 
to  aged  catecholamines.  After  exposure  to  fresh  cate- 
cholamines. larvae  rarely  showed  any  indication  of  velar 


1.0- 

0.9- 
0.8- 
0.7- 
0.6- 
0.5- 
0.4- 
0.3- 
0.2- 
0.1- 
0.0 


5 

T_ 


Dopamme       Norepmephrme     Epmephrme      Isoproterenol 


Figure  4.  Frequencies  of  velar  loss  after  7-h  exposure  to  fresh  (open 
bars)  or  aged  (hatched  bars)  solutions  of  catecholamines.  Concentrations 
are  2  x  10~4  M  except  for  dopamine  (I0~4  M).  Ordinate  values  and 
error  bars  are  means  and  standard  deviations,  respectively,  calculated 
from  arcsine  transformed  data.  Numbers  above  error  bars  indicate  the 
number  of  replicate  trials,  each  involving  a  chamber  containing  17-96 
larvae.  Trials  for  each  compound  were  conducted  on  at  least  two  different 
batches  of  larvae. 


314 


A.  PIRES  AND  M.  G.  HADFIELD 


0.2  0.3   0.4   05   06  0.7  08   09   1.0 
(Aged  Dopamme]  x  1 0  ^    (M) 

Figure  5.  Frequencies  of  velar  loss  after  7-h  exposure  to  varying 
concentrations  of  aged  dopamine  solutions.  Triangles  and  circles  represent 
two  assays  conducted  in  triplicate  on  two  different  batches  of  larvae. 
Each  symbol  represents  a  trial  involving  53-220  larvae.  Lines  connect 
grand  means  of  velar  loss  at  each  concentration,  calculated  from  arcsine 
transformed  data. 


loss  and  were  generally  indistinguishable  in  morphology 
and  behavior  from  untreated  animals.  Larvae  exposed  to 
a  higher  concentration  of  fresh  DA  (2  X  1(T4  M)  according 
to  this  protocol  did  sometimes  metamorphose  completely 
by  the  time  the  experiment  was  scored,  but  at  low  fre- 
quency (0-.25,  typically  .10-.  15).  as  suggested  by  earlier 
experiments  conducted  without  solution  replacement 
(Hadfield,  1984).  However,  in  the  current  work,  we  used 
1CT4  M  DA  for  the  aged  versus  fresh  comparison  because 
aged  solutions  at  higher  concentrations  proved  somewhat 
toxic  under  these  conditions.  Complete  metamorphosis 
was  observed  at  very  low  frequency  (<.05)  after  exposure 
to  fresh  1(T4MDA. 

The  frequency  of  velar  loss  in  response  to  varying  con- 
centrations of  aged  DA  is  given  in  Figure  5.  Concentration 
threshold  for  velar  loss  after  7-h  exposure  to  fresh  DA 
appears  to  lie  between  .25  and  .5  X  10~4  M. 

Experiments  to  test  morphogenic  effects  of  other  cat- 
echol  compounds  yielded  similar  results  (Fig.  6).  Aged 
solutions  of  the  deaminated  catecholamine  metabolites 
DOPAC  or  DOM  A  (both  2  X  1(T4  A/)  as  well  as  of  DOB 
(10~4  M)  consistently  yielded  high  frequencies  of  velar 
loss.  Fresh  solutions  of  DOPAC  and  DOMA  were  rela- 
tively ineffective.  Fresh  DOB  caused  most  larvae  to  show 
some  evidence  of  velar  loss,  but  this  was  invariably  con- 
fined to  the  loss  of  a  few  large  ciliated  cells  at  the  velar 
margin.  Aged  DOB,  in  contrast,  nearly  always  resulted  in 
detachment  of  all  the  ciliated  velar  cells  and  substantial 
regression  of  the  velar  lobes.  DOPA,  a  catechol  amino 
acid  precursor  of  the  catecholamine  neurotransmitters, 
also  caused  a  high  mean  frequency  of  velar  loss  (.67)  in 
aged  10~4  M  solutions,  but  a  quantitative  comparison 
could  not  be  made  with  fresh  solutions.  Larvae  treated 
with  fresh  DOPA  tended  to  withdraw  completely  into  the 
shell,  and  although  vela  appeared  to  be  intact,  accurate 
scoring  of  velar  condition  was  not  possible.  OCT  (NE 


1.0- 
0.9- 
0.8- 

07- 

0.6 

0.5 

04 

0.3 

0.2 

0.1 

00 


T 


i 


6 
T 


DOPAC 


DOMA 


DOB 


Figure  6.  Frequencies  of  velar  loss  after  7-h  exposure  to  fresh  (open 
bars)  or  aged  (hatched  bars)  solutions  of  the  catechol  compounds  dihy- 
droxyphenylacetic  acid  (DOPAC).  dihydroxymandelic  acid  (DOMA). 
and  dihydroxybenzene  (DOB).  Concentrations  are  2  X  1CT4  At  except 
for  DOB  ( 10~4  A/).  Ordinate  values  and  error  bars  are  means  and  standard 
deviations,  respectively,  calculated  from  arcsine  transformed  data. 
Numbers  above  error  bars  indicate  the  number  of  replicate  trials,  each 
involving  a  chamber  containing  18-112  larvae.  Trials  for  each  compound 
were  conducted  on  at  least  two  different  batches  of  larvae. 


minus  one  ring  hydroxyl  group)  and  HVA  (DOPAC  with 
one  ring  hydroxyl  group  methylated)  do  not  oxidize  as 
easily  as  their  related  catechol  compounds,  and  had  no 
morphogenic  effects  in  aged  or  fresh  2  X  1CT4  M  solutions. 

Quantification  of  H2O2-induced  velar  loss  and  abolition 
of  effects  ofH2O:  and  aged  catechols  by  catalase 

Because  autoxidation  of  catechols  in  water  yields  H2O2 
(Graham  ct  at..  1978).  we  tested  the  ability  of  H2O2  in 
ASW  solutions  to  induce  velar  loss.  Exposure  to  H2O2  for 
7  h  reliably  induced  velar  loss  at  a  concentration  threshold 
in  the  range  of  .2S-.5  X  1(T4  A/ (Fig.  7).  Solutions  of  .25 
X  10~4  M  H2O2  were  never  sufficient  to  cause  observable 
velar  loss;  these  animals  were  indistinguishable  from  un- 
treated individuals  (Fig.  3A).  A  large  but  variable  fraction 


1  On 


Figure  7.  Frequencies  of  velar  loss  after  7-h  exposure  to  varying 
concentrations  of  H2O2 .  Triangles,  circles  and  squares  represent  three 
assays  conducted  on  three  different  batches  of  larvae.  Each  symbol  rep- 
resents a  trial  involving  54-123  larvae.  Lines  connect  grand  means  of 
velar  loss  at  each  concentration,  calculated  from  arcsine  transformed 
data. 


MORPHOGENIC  EFFECT  OF 


315 


of  larvae  tested  at  .5  X  10  4  M  H2O2  showed  clear  indi- 
cations of  partial  velar  loss.  In  ICT4  M  H2O2  nearly  all 
larvae  experienced  at  least  partial  velar  loss;  a  typical  in- 
stance is  shown  in  Figure  3B.  In  2  X  ICT4  M  H2O2,  most 
larvae  lost  the  entire  velum  except  for  small  mounds  of 
cephalic  supportive  cells  (Fig.  3C). 

Morphogenic  potencies  of  H2O2  and  aged  solutions  of 
all  of  the  above  catechol  compounds  were  completely- 
abolished  by  10  min  incubation  with  purified  bovine  cat- 
alase  (5  ^g/ml),  prior  to  addition  of  larvae.  The  presence 
of  H2O2  in  aged  solutions  of  catechols  was  confirmed  by 
measuring  an  increase  in  dissolved  oxygen  concentration 
upon  catalase  treatment,  with  a  Clark-type  oxygen  meter. 
(Catalase  catalyzes  the  decomposition  of  H2O2  to  water 
and  molecular  oxygen.)  Hydrogen  peroxide-induced  velar 
loss  was  not  inhibited  by  acetylsalicylic  acid  (aspirin)  in 
any  concentration  between  10~h  and  10~3  M.  [Aspirin. 
an  inhibitor  of  prostaglandin  endoperoxide  synthetase. 
inhibits  H:O;-induced  spawning  in  the  abalone  Haliotix 
nifescens  (Morse  et  a/..  1977).] 

Discussion 

Relationship  oj  velar  loss  induced  by  H:O:  and  hy  aged 
catechols  to  velar  loss  in  natural  metamorphosis 

Velar  loss  in  larvae  of  P.  sibogae  can  be  induced  by 
application  of  H2O:  or  aged  solutions  of  catechols  in  the 
tenth-millimolar  concentration  range  (Figs.  3-7).  The 
stoichiometry  of  catechol  autoxidation  (Fig.  1),  together 
with  our  observation  that  similar  concentrations  of  H2O2 
or  aged  catechols  are  required  to  induce  velar  loss,  suggests 
the  hypothesis  that  morphogenic  activity  of  aged  catechols 
is  due  to  H2O2  produced  upon  autoxidation  or  to  some 
other  reactive  species  derived  from  H2O2  such  as  the  hy- 
droxyl  radical  HO'  (for  discussions  of  H2O2  metabolism 
and  oxygen  radical  biochemistry  see  Fridovich,  1978;  Im- 
lay  and  Linn,  1988;  Cadenas,  1989;  Kontos,  1989;  Gut- 
teridge  el  ul.,  1990).  Direct  evidence  for  this  hypothesis 
is  the  fact  that  morphogenic  activity  of  aged  catechol  so- 
lutions is  lost  on  incubation  with  catalase.  an  enzyme  that 
selectively  degrades  H2O2  to  molecular  oxygen  and  water. 
However,  we  have  not  yet  rigorously  excluded  cooperative 
effects  of  quinone  oxidation  products  of  catechols  in  velar 
destruction. 

Natural  coral-induced  metamorphosis  is  preceded  by 
settlement  behavior  in  which  the  larva  takes  up  a  char- 
acteristic posture,  attached  by  the  foot  to  the  substratum 
(BonarandHadfield,  1974;  Hadfield,  1978).  This  behavior 
is  not  elicited  by  H2O2  or  aged  catechols.  In  natural  meta- 
morphosis, velar  loss  ensues  in  this  settled  position.  Larvae 
treated  with  H2O2  or  aged  catechols  begin  to  lose  the 
velum  while  swimming;  after  enough  large  ciliated  velar 
cells  have  been  lost,  larvae  sink  to  the  bottom  of  the  ex- 


perimental chamber  and  typically  lie  on  a  side  of  the  shell 
in  an  extended  posture. 

Although  the  behavioral  contexts  for  natural  and  ar- 
tificially induced  velar  loss  are  different,  the  morphological 
phenomena  share  several  common  features.  Both  begin 
with  the  detachment  of  the  large  ciliated  cells  at  the  velar 
margin.  Natural  and  artificially  induced  velar  loss  are  both 
highly  tissue-specific  in  that  cell  separation  and  tissue 
regression  are  confined  to  the  velum  and  are  not  mani- 
fested in  other  ciliated  epithelia.  Following  loss  of  the  cil- 
iated velar  cells,  clumps  of  nonciliated  supportive  cells 
remain  as  cephalic  mounds  (Fig.  3C).  Partial  metamor- 
phosis induced  by  H2O2  or  aged  catechols  does  not  pro- 
ceed beyond  this  point.  However,  no  loss  of  metamorphic 
competence  has  occurred,  because  such  larvae  can  resume 
metamorphosis  once  exposed  to  CI. 

Further  experiments  are  required  to  test  the  hypothesis 
that  H2O2  or  derivative  oxygen  radicals  mediate  velar  loss 
in  natural  metamorphosis.  Several  techniques  exist  that 
are  potentially  applicable  to  detection  of  H2O2  or  oxygen 
radical  production  in  metamorphosing  tissues  (Freeman 
and  Crapo,  1981;  Radzik  el  ai.  1983;  Ruch  et  ai.  1983; 
Kontos,  1989).  Chemical  scavengers  of  free  radicals  and 
of  H2O2  can  also  be  used  to  interfere  with  radical-depen- 
dent mechanisms  (Cadenas,  1989;  Kontos,  1989).  We 
were  unable  to  inhibit  coral-induced  metamorphosis  with 
catalase,  but  that  result  is  difficult  to  interpret  because 
catalase  is  not  expected  to  penetrate  cells  to  reach  potential 
sites  of  endogenous  H2O2  generation  or  action.  Future 
experiments  will  include  other  more  penetrant  scavengers. 
It  is  important  to  note  that  even  if  H2O2  or  oxygen  radicals 
are  implicated  in  the  mechanism  of  natural  metamor- 
phosis, there  are  many  biological  sources  of  these  oxygen 
species  other  than  oxidation  of  catechols  (Cohen,  1983; 
Kontos,  1989). 

Possible  modes  of  action  ofH:O: 

Hydrogen  peroxide  is  well-known  for  its  cytotoxic 
properties,  which  render  it  useful  as  a  topical  disinfectant. 
Mechanisms  of  cell  damage  by  H2O2  and  oxygen  radicals 
have  been  subjects  of  several  recent  discussions  (Imlay 
and  Linn,  1988;  Kontos,  1989;  Gutteridge  and  Halliwell, 
1990).  Indeed,  significant  lysis  of  certain  cultured  verte- 
brate epithelial  cells  occurs  upon  exposure  to  H2O2  con- 
centrations only  slightly  higher  than  those  demonstrated 
to  cause  velar  loss  in  the  present  study  (Hayden  et  ai, 
1990;  Polansky  et  ai,  1990).  However,  the  question  of 
whether  H2O2-induced  velar  disintegration  in  Phestilla 
larvae  is  a  cytotoxic  response  remains.  The  facts  that  cil- 
iated cells  appear  to  be  shed  intact  with  cilia  still  beating 
and  that  other  epithelial  tissues  show  no  evidence  of  injury 
at  morphogenically  active  concentrations,  might  argue 
otherwise.  Close  structural  examination  of  shed  velar  cells 


316 


A.  PIRES  AND  M.  G.  HADFIELD 


in  both  H:O:-induced  and  coral-induced  velar  loss  will 
be  instructive  in  this  regard.  Non-toxic  regulatory  effects 
of  H:O:  mimic  the  effects  of  insulin  on  glucose,  carbo- 
hydrate, and  lipid  metabolism  in  vertebrate  cells,  possibly 
by  stimulating  phosphorylation  of  the  insulin  receptor 
(Heffetz  el  a/..  1990).  This  mechanism  of  action  may  war- 
rant investigation  in  Phestilla.  particularly  in  light  of  the 
recent  discovery  of  a  preproinsulin-related  peptide  in 
growth-regulating  neuroendocrine  cells  of  the  gastropod 
Lymnaea  stagnalis  (Smit  et  ai,  1988)  and  the  growing 
appreciation  of  insulin's  role  in  cellular  differentiation 
during  embryogenesis  (Alemany  et  a/.,  1990).  In  another 
gastropod,  the  abalone  H.  ntfescens.  H:O2  induces 
spawning,  probably  by  activating  prostaglandin  endoper- 
oxide  synthetase  (Morse  et  ai,  1977).  This  particular 
pathway  is  unlikely  to  be  involved  in  velar  loss  in  Phestilla 
because  H^Oi  induction  of  velar  loss  is  not  blocked  by 
aspirin,  a  potent  inhibitor  of  that  enzyme.  One  might 
speculate  that  FLO:  could  also  regulate  the  activity  of  a 
factor  involved  in  epithelial  cell  adhesive  interactions, 
perhaps  akin  to  the  "scatter  factor"  recently  described  as 
a  promoter  of  cell-cell  separation  in  cultured  mammalian 
epithelia  (Stoker  and  Gherardi,  1989). 

Implications  for  other  taxa 

Our  study  points  out  the  need  for  caution  in  the  inter- 
pretation of  behavioral  and  morphogenic  effects  of  bath- 
applied  catecholamines  on  marine  animals.  In  the  bivalve 
Crassostrea  gigas.  sufficient  physical  controls  and  cor- 
roborating pharmacological  evidence  have  been  mar- 
shalled in  support  of  the  hypothesis  that  dopaminergic 
and  adrenergic  neural  pathways,  mediate  settlement  and 
morphogenesis,  respectively  (Coon  and  Bonar,  1987; 
Bonar  et  a/..  1990).  There  are  also  brief  reports  of  meta- 
morphic  induction  by  DOPA  in  the  mussel  Mytilits  eclulis 
(Cooper,  1982),  and  by  DA  in  the  mud  snail  Ilyanassa 
obsoleta  (Levantine  and  Bonar,  1986),  but  details  of  the 
methods  are  not  given.  Certain  catecholamines  have  been 
reported  to  induce  metamorphosis  in  the  scallops  Patin- 
opecten  yessoensis  (Kingzett  et  a/..  1990)  and  Pecten 
maximus  (Cochard  el  al.,  1 989),  but  in  both  of  these  stud- 
ies the  problem  of  catechol  oxidation  was  not  thoroughly 
resolved.  DOPA  induces  a  low  frequency  of  metamor- 
phosis in  the  polychaete  Phragmatopoma  ealifornica 
(Jensen,  1987),  but  that  work  has  implicated  cross-linked 
quinoid  derivatives  of  DOPA  residues  in  proteins,  rather 
than  catecholamine  neurotransmitters,  in  the  inductive 
pathway  (Jensen  and  Morse,  1990).  To  our  knowledge 
the  only  other  published  example  of  catecholamine  in- 
duction of  larval  metamorphosis  is  in  the  echinoid  Den- 
draster  excentricits  (Burke,  1983).  Whole  larvae  and  ex- 
cised larval  arms  metamorphosed  in  response  to  DA,  but 
not  to  EP  or  NE.  The  response  specificity  suggests  that 


DA  did  not  act  as  a  source  of  H:O:  in  those  experiments, 
but  the  protocols  are  not  detailed  enough  to  permit  clear 
resolution  of  this  issue. 

Our  results  should  not  be  interpreted  to  mean  that  cat- 
echolamines do  not  act  as  neurotransmitters  mediating 
metamorphosis  in  P.  sibogae.  Bath-applied  substances 
might  not  reach  their  target  tissues  in  the  proper  concen- 
tration, or  the  co-activation  of  receptors  on  many  cells 
throughout  the  nervous  system  may  result  in  net  inhibi- 
tion of  circuits  that  effect  metamorphosis.  We  do  hope 
that  this  study  sounds  a  cautionary  note  to  others  inves- 
tigating chemical  control  of  metamorphosis,  and  prompts 
consideration  of  a  possible  morphogenic  role  for  H2O;. 

Acknowledgments 

We  thank  Esther  Leise  for  critically  reading  the  manu- 
script, and  Leonard  Deal  for  competent  and  reliable  tech- 
nical assistance.  Supported  by  NSF  grant  DCB89-03800 
to  M.G.H. 

Literature  Cited 

Alemany,  J.,  M.  Girbau.  and  F.  de  Pablo.  1990.  Insulin  action  and 
insulin  receptors  in  embryogenesis  of  vertebrates  and  invertebrates. 
Pp.  198-204  in  Progress  in  Comparative  Endocrinology:  Proceedings 
of  the  Eleventh  International  Symposium  on  Comparative  Endocri- 
nology, A.  Epple,  C.  G.  Scanes,  and  M.  H.  Stetson,  eds.  Wiley-Liss. 
New  York. 

Bonar,  D.  B.,  S.  L.  Coon,  M.  Walch,  R.  M.  Weiner,  and  \V.  Fitt. 
1990.  Control  of  oyster  settlement  and  metamorphosis  by  endog- 
enous and  exogenous  chemical  cues.  Bull.  Mar  Sci.  46:  484-498. 

Bonar,  D.  B.,  and  M.  G.  Hadfield.  1974.  Metamorphosis  of  the  marine 
gastropod  Phextilla  sibogae  Bergh  ( Nudibranchia:  Aeolidacea).  I.  Light 
and  electron  microscopic  analysis  of  larval  and  metamorphic  stages. 
J.  Exp.  Mar.  Biol.  Ecol.  16:  227-255. 

Burke,  R.  D.  1983.  Neural  control  of  metamorphosis  in  Dendraster 
exccninciis  Biol  Bull.  164:  176-188. 

Cadenas,  F.  1989.  Biochemistry  of  oxygen  toxicity.  Ann.  Rev  Biochem. 
58:79-110. 

Cavanaugh,  G.  M.  1956.  Formulae  and  Methods  17  of  I  he  Marine 
Biological  Laboratory  Chemical  Room  Marine  Biological  Laboratory. 
Woods  Hole.  Massachusetts. 

Cochard,  J.  C.,  L.  Chevolot,  J.  C.  Yvin,  and  A.  M.  Chevolot-Mageur. 
1989.  Induction  de  la  metamorphose  de  la  coquille  Saint  Jacques 
Pecten  maximus  L.  par  des  derives  de  la  tyrosine  extraits  de  1'algue 
Delesseria  sanguinea  Lamouroux  ou  synthetiques.  Hahtotis  19:  259- 
274. 

Cohen,  G.  1983.  The  pathobiology  of  Parkinson's  disease:  biochemical 
aspects  of  dopamine  neuron  senescence.  /  Neural  Transmission, 
Suppl.  19:89-103. 

Coon,  S.  L.,  and  D.  B.  Bonar.  1987.  Pharmacological  evidence  that 
alpha- 1  adrenoceptors  mediate  metamorphosis  ol  the  Pacific  oyster, 
Crassostrea  gigas.  Neuroscience  23:  1 169-1 174. 

Cooper,  K.  1982.  A  model  to  explain  the  induction  of  settlement  and 
metamorphosis  of  planktonic  eyed-pediveligers  of  the  blue  mussel 
Mytilus  edulis  L.  by  chemical  and  tactile  cues.  J.  Shellfish  Res.  2: 
117. 

Freeman,  B.  A.,  and  J.  D.  Crapo.  1981.  Hyperoxia  increases  oxygen 
radical  production  in  rat  lungs  and  lung  mitochondria. ./  Biol.  Chem. 
256:  10986-10992. 


MORPHOGEN1C  EFFECT  OF  H:O, 


317 


Fridovich,  I.  1978.     The  biology  of  oxygen  radicals.  Science  201:  875- 

880. 
Graham,  D.  G.  1978.     Oxidative  pathways  for  catecholamines  in  the 

genesis  of  neuromelanin  and  cytotoxic  quinones.  Mol  Pharmacol 

14:  633-643. 
Graham,  D.  G.,  S.  M.  Tiffany,  VV.  R.  J.  Bell,  and  \V.  F.  Gutknechl. 

1978.     Autoxidation  versus  covalent  binding  of  quinones  as  the 

mechanism  of  toxicity  of  dopamine.  6-hydroxydopamine.  and  related 

compounds  toward  C1300  neuroblastoma  cells  in  vitro  Mol  f'/nu- 

nnicol  14:  644-653. 
Gutteridge,  J.  M.  C.,  and  B.  Halliwell.  1990.     The  measurement  and 

mechanism  of  lipid  peroxidation  in  biological  systems.  TIBS  15: 

129-135. 
Gutteridge,   J.   M.   C.,   I.   Zs.-Nagy,   L.   Maidt,   and    R.    A.   Floyd. 

1990.     ADP-iron  as  a  Fenton  reactant:  radical  reactions  detected  by 

spin  trapping,  hydrogen  abstraction,  and  aromatic  hydroxylation. 

Arch  Biochem  Biophys.  Ill:  422-428. 
Hadtield,  M.  G.  1978.     Metamorphosis  in  marine  molluscan  larvae:  an 

analysis  of  stimulus  and  response.  Pp.  165-175  in  Settlement  and 

Metamorphosis  ol  Marine  Invertebrate  Larvae.  F.-S.  Chia  and  M. 

Rice.  eds.  Elsevier/North-Holland,  New  York 
Itadfield,  M.  G.  1984.     Settlement  requirements  of  molluscan  larvae: 

new  data  on  chemical  and  genetic  roles.  Atinuciilture  39:  283-298. 
Hadfield,  M.  G.  1986.     Settlement  and  recruitment  of  marine  inver- 
tebrates: a  perspective  and  some  proposals.  Bull  Mar  Sci  39:  4 1 8- 

425. 
Hadfield,  M.  G.,  and  J.  T.  Pennington.  1990.     Nature  of  the  metamor- 

phic  signal  and  its  internal  transduction  in  larvae  of  the  nudibranch 

Phestilla  sihogtie  Bull  Mar  Set.  46:  455-464. 
Hawley,  M.  D.,  S.  V.  Tatawawadi,  S.  Pickarski,  and  R.  N.  Adams. 

1967.     Electrochemical  studies  of  the  oxidation  pathways  of  cate- 
cholamines. J  Am  Chem  Soc  89:  447-450. 
Haydcn,  B.  J.,  L.  Zhu,  D.  Sens,  M.  J.  Tapert,  and  R.  K.  Crouch. 

1990.     Cytolysis  of  corneal  epithelial  cells  by  hydrogen  peroxide. 

E.\p.  Eye  Res.  50:  11-16. 
Heacock,  R.  A.  1959.     The  chemistry  of  adrenochrome  and  related 

compounds.  Chem.  Rev  59:  181-237. 
Heffetz,  D..  I.  Bushkin,  R.  Dror,  and  V.  Zick.  1990.     The  insulinomi- 

metic  agents  HiCK  and  vanadate  stimulate  protein  tyrosine  phos- 

phorylation  in  intact  cells.  J.  Biol.  Chem.  265:  2896-2902. 
Mirata,  K.  Y.,  and  M.  G.  Hadfield.  1986.     The  role  of  choline  in  meta- 

morphic  induction  of  Phestilla  (Gastropoda:  Nudibranchia).  J  Comp. 

Biochem.  Physiol.  84C:  15-21. 
Imlay,  J.  A.,  and  S.  Linn.  1988.     DNA  damage  and  oxygen  radical 

toxicity.  Seienee  240:  1302-1309. 
Jensen,  R.  A.  1987.     Factors  affecting  the  settlement,  metamorphosis 

and  distribution  of  larvae  of  the  marine  polychaete  Phragmalopoithi 


ciilifarnica  (Fewkes).  Ph.D.  dissertation.  University  of  California, 
Santa  Barbara,  California.  175  pp. 

Jensen,  R.  A.,  and  D.  E.  Morse.  1990.  Chemically  induced  metamor- 
phosis of  polychaete  larvae  in  both  the  laboratory  and  ocean  envi- 
ronment. /  Chem  Ecol  16:911-430. 

Kempf,  S.  C.,  and  M.  G.  Hadfield.  1985.  Planktotrophy  by  the  lecitho- 
trophic  larvae  of  a  nudibranch,  Phe\lilla  sibogae  (Gastropoda).  Biol 
Bull.  169:  119-130. 

Kingzetl,  B.  C.,  N.  Bourne,  and  K.  l.eask.  1990.  Induction  of  meta- 
morphosis of  the  Japanese  scallop  Potinopecten  yessoensis  Jay.  J. 
Shellfish  Res.  9:  119-124. 

Kontos,  H.  A.  1989.  Oxygen  radicals  in  CNS  damage.  Chem. -Biol 
Interactions  72:  229-255. 

Levantine,  P.  L.,  and  D.  B.  Bonar.  1986.  Metamorphosis  ofllyanassa 
obso/ela:  natural  and  artihcial  inducers.  Am.  Zoo/.  26:  14A. 

Miller,  S.  E.,  and  M.  G.  Hadfield.  1986.  Ontogeny  of  phototaxis  and 
metamorphic  competence  in  larvae  of  the  nudibranch  Phestilla  si- 
bogae  Bergh  (Gastropoda:  Opisthobranchia).  J  Exp.  Mar.  Biol.  Ecol. 
97:95-112. 

Misra,  H.  P.,  and  I.  Fridovich.  1972.  The  role  of  superoxide  anion  in 
the  autoxidation  of  epinephrine  and  a  simple  assay  for  superoxide 
dismutase.  J.  Biol  Chem  247:  3170-3175. 

Morse,  D.  E.,  H.  Duncan,  N.  Hooker,  and  A.  Morse.  1977.  Hydrogen 
peroxide  induces  spawning  in  mollusks,  with  activation  of  prosta- 
glandin  endoperoxide  synthetase.  Science  196:  298-300. 

Pennington,  J.  T.,  and  M.  G.  Hadfield.  1989.  Larvae  of  a  nudibranch 
mollusc  ( Pheslilla  sihogac)  metamorphose  when  exposed  to  common 
organic  solvents.  Biol.  Bull.  177:  350-355. 

Polansky,  J.  R.,  D.  J.  Fauss,  T.  Hydorn,  and  E.  Bloom.  1990.  Cellular 
injury  from  sustained  r.v.  acute  hydrogen  peroxide  exposure  in  cul- 
tured human  corneal  endothelium  and  human  lens  epithelium. 
C  LAO  J.  16(1  Suppl.):  S23-S28. 

Radzik,  D.  M.,  D.  A.  Roston,  and  P.  I .  Kissinger.  1983.  Determination 
of  hydro.xylated  aromatic  compounds  produced  via  superoxide-de- 
pendent  formation  of  hydroxyl  radicals  by  liquid  chromatography/ 
electrochemistry.  Anal.  Biochem.  131:  458-464. 

Ruch,  W.,  P.  H.  Cooper,  and  M.  Baggiolini.  1983.  Assay  of  H:O2  pro- 
duction by  macrophages  and  neutrophils  with  homovanillic  acid  and 
horse-radish  peroxidase.  /  Immunol  Methods 63:  347-357. 

Smit,  A.  B.,  E.  Vrenghdenhil.  R.  H.  M.  Ebberink,  W.  P.  M.  Geraerts, 
J.  RIootHijk,  and  J.  Joose.  1988.  Growth-controlling  molluscan 
neurons  produce  the  precursor  of  an  insulin-related  peptide.  Nature 
331:  535-538. 

Stoker,  M.,  and  E.  Gherardi.  1989.  Scatter  factor  and  other  regulators 
of  cell  mobility.  Br  Med.  Bull.  45:  481-491. 

Yool,  A.  J.,  S.  M.  Grau,  M.  G.  Hadfield,  R.  A.  Jensen,  D.  A.  Markell, 
and  D.  E.  Morse.  1986.  Excess  potassium  induces  larval  meta- 
morphosis in  four  marine  invertebrate  species.  Biol.  Bull.  170:  255- 
266. 


Reference:  Biol.  Bid!  180:  318-327.  (April,  1991) 


cDNA  Sequences  Reveal  mRNAs  for  Two  Ga  Signal 
Transducing  Proteins  from  Larval  Cilia 

LISA  M.  WODICKA  AND  DANIEL  E.  MORSE 

Department  of  Biological  Sciences  and  the  Marine  Biotechnology  Center, 
University  of  California,  Santa  Barbara,  California  93106 


Abstract.  In  planktonic  larvae  of  the  gastropod  mollusk, 
H allot  is  nifescens  (red  abalone),  settlement  behavior  and 
subsequent  metamorphosis  are  controlled  by  two  con- 
vergent chemosensory  pathways  that  report  unique  pep- 
tide  and  amino  acid  signals  from  the  environment.  The 
integration  of  signals  from  these  two  sensory  pathways 
provides  for  variable  amplification,  or  fine-tuning,  of  larval 
responsiveness  to  the  inducers  of  settlement  and  meta- 
morphosis. These  pathways  may  be  analogous  to  the  neu- 
ronal  and  molecular  mechanisms  of  facilitation  and  long- 
term  potentiation  characterized  in  other  (adult)  molluscan 
systems.  Recently,  the  chemosensory  receptors  and  signal 
transducers  apparently  belonging  to  the  regulatory  path- 
way (including  a  G  protein  and  protein  kinase  C)  have 
been  identified  in  cilia  purified  from  H.  nifescens  larvae. 
These  elements  retain  their  sequential  receptor-dependent 
regulation  in  the  isolated  cilia  in  vitro.  As  a  first  step  toward 
the  molecular  genetic  dissection  of  the  receptors,  trans- 
ducers, and  the  mechanisms  of  their  control  of  settlement 
behavior  and  metamorphosis,  we  present  evidence  that 
the  cilia  purified  from  these  larvae  contain  polyadenylated 
mRNA  corresponding  to  unique  signal  transducers.  Pu- 
rification of  this  mRNA,  enzymatic  synthesis  of  the  cor- 
responding cDNAs,  amplification  by  the  polymerase  chain 
reaction,  cloning,  and  sequence  analysis  reveal  that  the 
ciliary  mRNA  includes  sequences  that  apparently  code 
for  two  Ga  signal  transducing  proteins.  One  of  these  is 
highly  homologous  to  members  of  the  Gq  family,  recently 


Received  9  July  1990;  accepted  28  January  1991. 

1  Abbreviations:  GITC.  guanidinium  isothiocyanate:  SDS.  sodium 
dodecyl  sulfate;  IPTG.  isopropyl-fMhiogalactoside;  X-gal,  bromo-,  chloro- 
indolylgalactoside:  AMV,  avian  myeloblastosis  virus;  Tris,  tris-hydroxy- 
methylaminomethane;  EDTA.  ethylenediamine-tetra-acetic  acid;  TE, 
Tris-EDTA;  TBE,  Tris-borate-EDTA;  TAE.  Tris-acetate-EDTA;  bp,  base- 
pair(s).  The  standard  one-letter  amino  acid  code  is  used. 


shown  in  other  systems  to  control  the  activity  of  phos- 
pholipase  C;  the  other  is  more  closely  related  to  G,  and 
G0.  These  results  extend  the  tractability  of  the  Haliotis 
system  to  analyses  of  cDNA  and  protein  sequences  of 
chemosensory  elements  from  isolated  cilia.  This  is  the 
first  time  that  mRNA  has  been  purified  from  isolated  cilia, 
and  the  corresponding  cDNA  synthesized  and  character- 
ized. 

Introduction 

Larvae  of  the  gastropod  mollusk,  Haliotis  nifescens  (red 
abalone),  undergo  a  dramatic  behavioral  change  when 
they  encounter  a  specific  chemical  cue  at  the  surfaces  of 
crustose  red  algae:  the  planktonic  larvae  cease  swimming, 
attach  to  the  algal  surface,  and  commence  metamorphosis 
and  plantigrade  locomotion  and  feeding.  This  behavioral 
transition  is  controlled  by  the  integration  of  two  conver- 
gent chemosensory  pathways  that  respond  to  chemical 
signals  from  the  environment:  a  morphogenetic  pathway 
activated  by  a  GABA-mimetic  morphogen  encountered 
by  the  larvae  on  surfaces  of  recruiting  algae,  and  a  regu- 
latory or  amplifier  pathway  stimulated  by  lysine  in  sea- 
water  (Morse  et  al.,  1984;  Trapido-Rosenthal  and  Morse, 
1985,  1986a,  b;  Baxter  and  Morse,  1987;  Morse  1990a). 
Activation  of  the  morphogenetic  pathway  receptors  is 
thought  to  trigger  an  efflux  of  chloride  or  other  anions 
across  the  membrane  of  the  primary  chemosensory  cell, 
apparently  resulting  in  excitatory  depolarization  (Baloun 
and  Morse.  1984).  This  transduction  of  the  exogenous 
chemical  signal  to  one  that  can  be  propagated  by  the  larval 
nervous  system  is  evidently  sufficient  to  induce  the  change 
in  larval  behavior  culminating  in  settlement,  attachment, 
and  the  start  of  metamorphosis.  Activation  of  the  amplifier 
pathway  receptors  increases  the  sensitivity  or  output  of 
the  morphogenetic  pathway  by  as  much  as  100-fold.  The 


318 


Ga  CDNAS  FROM   LARVAL  CILIA 


319 


receptors  of  the  amplifier  pathway  are  activated  when  they 
bind  lysine,  lysine  polymers,  or  certain  lysine  analogs 
(Trapido-Rosenthal  and  Morse,  1985.  1986b).  Experi- 
ments //;  vivo  demonstrated  that  the  amplifier  pathway  is 
controlled  by  chemosensory  receptors  and  signal  trans- 
ducers distinct  from  those  of  the  morphogenetic  pathway, 
and  that  the  lysine  receptors  of  the  amplifier  pathway  ac- 
tivate a  sequential  G  protein-(phospholipase  C)  diacyl- 
glycerol-protein  kinase  C  signal  transduction  cascade 
(Baxter  and  Morse,  1987).  This  system  of  dual  control, 
in  which  the  integration  of  two  different  kinds  of  che- 
mosensory signals  from  the  environment  modulates  the 
settlement  behavior  of  the  Haliotis  larvae,  fine-tunes  larval 
responsiveness  to  exogenous  settlement  cues.  The  result 
of  this  integration  may  enhance  the  site-specificity  of  larval 
settlement  and  metamorphosis  in  potentially  favorable 
habitats  (Trapido-Rosenthal  and  Morse,  1985,  1986b: 
Morse,  1990a,  b). 

Because  both  the  morphogenetic  and  amplifier  path- 
ways can  be  activated  by  macromolecular  (protein-asso- 
ciated or  polypeptide)  ligands  that  are  presumably  im- 
permeant  (Morse  ct  a/..  1984;  Trapido-Rosenthal  and 
Morse.  1985.  1986b),  it  was  suspected  that  the  chemo- 
sensory receptors  controlling  these  two  pathways  might 
be  located  on  externally  accessible  epithelia  (Morse,  1985, 
1990a,  b).  Epithelial  cilia  are  known  to  carry  chemosen- 
sory receptors  in  a  wide  variety  of  systems,  including  the 
well-characterized  olfactory  epithelia  of  frogs,  fish,  and 
mammals  (e.g.,  Rhein  and  Cagan,  1980;  Chen  and  Lancet 
1984;  Pace  el  at.,  1985;  Pace  and  Lancet,  1986;  Lancet 
and  Pace,  1987;  Anholt,  1987;  Anholt  el  ai.  1987).  Epi- 
thelial cilia  also  have  long  been  suspected  to  carry  the 
chemosensory  structures  that  mediate  substratum  rec- 
ognition and  thereby  control  settlement  behavior  and 
metamorphosis  in  various  molluscan  larvae  (Raven,  1958: 
Fretter  and  Graham,  1962;  Bonar,  1978a,  b;  Chia  and 
Ross.  1984;  Yool,  1985). 

Recently,  epithelial  cilia  isolated  from  H.  rufescens  lar- 
vae were  shown  to  contain  the  lysine  receptors  and  signal 
transducers  that  may  control  the  amplifier  pathway  in 
vivo  (Baxter  and  Morse,  in  prep.).  These  elements  retain 
their  functional  coupling  in  the  isolated  cilia  in  vitro:  i.e., 
the  specific  and  saturable  binding  of  lysine  to  sodium- 
independent  lysine  receptors  activates  sequentially  a  G 
protein  and  diacylglycerol-stimulated  protein  kinase  C 
(Baxter,  1991;  Baxter  and  Morse,  in  prep.).  The  lysine- 
binding  receptor  was  found  to  be  reciprocally  regulated 
by  its  tightly  coupled  G  protein  in  the  cilia  in  vitro  (Baxter 
and  Morse,  in  prep.);  similar  behavior  is  exhibited  by  other 
members  of  the  rhodopsin  and  /3-adrenergic  G  protein- 
coupled  transmembrane  receptor  superfamily. 

The  tools  of  molecular  genetics  are  required  to  further 
resolve  the  mechanisms  by  which  the  chemosensory  and 
neuronal  receptors,  transducers,  and  pathways  are  inte- 


grated to  control  behavior  in  these  small  larvae  (Morse, 
1990a).  As  a  first  step  toward  that  objective,  we  report 
here  the  amplification,  cloning,  and  partial  sequence 
analysis  of  cDNAs  apparently  corresponding  to  two  Ga 
signal  transducing  proteins,  from  mRNA  purified  from 
the  isolated  cilia. 


Materials  and  Methods 


Cilia  isolation 


Larvae  of  Haliotis  rufescens  were  produced  in  the  lab- 
oratory by  hydrogen  peroxide-induced  spawning  of  gravid 
adults  (Morse  et  ai.  1977).  Larvae  were  maintained  at 
15°C  in  5  /urn-filtered,  UV-sterilized  running  seawater 
until  7  days  post-fertilization;  at  this  time  they  become 
developmental^  competent  to  metamorphose  in  response 
to  inducer  (Morse  et  ai.  1979,  1980).  Cilia  were  purified 
by  differential  centrifugation,  after  abscision  induced  by 
exposure  of  the  larvae  to  a  mild  calcium-ethanol  shock 
(Baxter  and  Morse,  in  prep.).  This  method  is  a  modifi- 
cation of  that  used  for  the  purification  of  functional  re- 
ceptor-bearing cilia  from  olfactory  epithelia  (Rhein  and 
Cagan,  1980;  Chen  and  Lancet,  1984)  and  other  sources 
(Watson  and  Hopkins,  1962;  Linck,  1973).  Electron  mi- 
crograph examination  reveals  the  purified  cilia  to  be  intact 
and  completely  free  of  cell  bodies  and  debris;  the  cilia  are 
heterogeneous,  and  include  short  (ca.  0.5  ^m)  spatulate 
cilia  similar  to  the  sensory  cilia  found  in  other  invertebrate 
systems,  and  long  (>  10  ^m)  propulsive  cilia  from  the  lar- 
val velum  (Baxter  and  Morse,  in  prep.). 

RNA  isolation 

We  isolated  total  RNA  from  cilia  freshly  purified  from 
Haliotis  rufescens  larvae,  using  a  single-step  extraction 
with  an  acid  guanidinium  isothiocyanate  (GITC1  (-phenol- 
chloroform  mixture  (Chomczynski  and  Sacchi,  1987). 
Poly  A+  mRNA  was  purified  using  oligo  (dT)  cellulose 
columns  either  centrifuged  (Clontech,  Palo  Alto,  Califor- 
nia) or  used  with  a  syringe  (Stratagene,  La  Jolla,  Califor- 
nia) in  a  modification  of  the  technique  described  by  Aviv 
and  Leder(  1972). 

RNA  was  purified  from  bovine  retina  to  provide  a  pos- 
itive control  enriched  for  G  protein  (transducin)  mRNA. 
For  this  purpose,  fresh  bovine  eyes  were  obtained  from 
Federal  Meat  Market  (Vernon,  California).  Retinas  were 
immediately  dissected  on  ice  in  Tris-buffered  saline 
(Maniatis  et  ai,  1982)  and  placed  on  dry  ice  for  transport 
to  a  nearby  laboratory.  There,  half  the  samples  were  frozen 
in  liquid  nitrogen,  and  half  were  homogenized  in  GITC; 
these  samples  then  were  transported  to  Santa  Barbara  in 
GITC  or  on  dry  ice.  RNA  was  isolated  by  the  GITC- 
cesium  chloride  centrifugation  method  (Chirgwin  et  ai, 
1979),  and  mRNA  was  then  purified  as  described  above. 


320 


L    M.  WODICRA  AND  D    E.  MORSE 


Total  RNA  from  both  sources  was  analyzed  by  elec- 
trophoresis  on  formaldehyde  gels  (Maniatis  el  at.  1982) 
with  ethidium  bromide  added  to  the  sample  buffer,  or 
was  denatured  at  65 °C  for  1 5  min.  quickly  chilled  on  ice. 
electrophoresed  on  1%  agarose/TBE  gels  (Han  el  at.  1987) 
and  visualized  by  U  V-excited  fluorescence  after  ethidium 
bromide  staining  (Maniatis  el  at.  1982).  Purity  of  total 
and  poly  A+  RNA  was  confirmed  by  the  ratio  of  absor- 
bances  at  260  and  280  nm  and  concentrations  estimated 
from  the  A2W>  • 

Synthesis  ofcDNA  and  oligonucleotide  primers 

Reverse  transcriptase  from  avian  myeloblastosis  virus 
( AMV)  (Invitrogen.  San  Diego,  CA)  was  used  to  synthesize 
first  strand  cDNA.  Cilia  mRNA  (100-500  ng)  or  bovine 
mRNA  ( 1  Mg)  was  used  for  each  50  jul  reaction. 

For  polymerase  chain  reaction  (PCR)  amplifications, 
two  kinds  of  oligonucleotide  primers  were  made:  degen- 
erate primers  (D)  were  used  for  the  first  amplifications  of 
the  cDNA,  and  (once  the  exact  sequence  of  the  Ga  cDNA 
was  determined)  specific  primers  (S)  were  used  to  amplify 
the  genomic  sequences  from  sperm  DNA.  Oligonucleo- 
tides  used  as  primers  for  PCR  were  synthesized  (as  the 
trityl-derivatives)  by  an  automated  oligonucleotide  syn- 
thesizer (Applied  Biosystems  Inc.,  Foster  City,  California). 
To  reduce  the  degeneracy  of  the  primers,  Haliolis  rufes- 
cens  condon  usage  frequencies  (Groppe  and  Morse,  1989) 
were  taken  into  consideration,  and  two  separate  pools  of 
the  downstream  primer  were  synthesized  (D2  and  D, ). 
Degenerate    oligonucleotide    primer    sequences    corre- 
sponding to  the  conserved  G  and  G'  domains  (Lochrie 
and  Simon,  1988)  of  G  protein  a  subunits  are:  D,:  5' 
GAAGGATCCAAGTGGATCCA(GC)TG(CT)TTT    3': 
D,:  5'  CTCAAGCTTTCCT(TG)CTT(AG)TT(TG)AG- 
(AG)AA  3':  D3:  5'  CTCAAGCTTTCTT(TG)CTT(AG)- 
TT(CA)AG(AG)AA  3'.  D,  corresponds  to  the  conserved 
G'  domain  amino  acid  sequence,  KWI(HQ)CF;  D2  and 
D,  correspond  to  the  conserved  G  domain  sequence 
FLNK(KQ)D.  Amino  acid  sequences  chosen  for  these 
domains  were  based  on  the  findings  of  Strathmann  el  at 
(1989),  with  inclusion  of  a  degeneracy  representing  ad- 
ditional sequences  determined  for  yeast.  In  addition,  these 
primers  include  oligonucleotide  sequences  (indicated  by 
underlining)  corresponding  to  the  BamH  I  (D, )  and  Hind 
III  (D2  and  D, )  restriction  enzyme  targets;  these  sequences 
were  added  as  linkers  to  facilitate  cloning  of  the  amplified 
products.  A  shorter  variant  of  D,  also  was  produced  with- 
out the  9-nucleotide  linker.  Specific  (non-degenerate) 
primer  sequences  based  on  the  cDNA  sequence  subse- 
quently determined  for  the  Haliolis  cilia  G«l  (see  below) 
are:  S, :  5'  GCAGGATCCACGTCCATCATGTTCTTA 
3';     and     S2:     5'     CTCAAGCTTCGGGTAGGTGA- 
TAATCGT  3'.  These  primers  also  have  9-nucleotide  long 
5'-linkers  with  a  BamH  1  site  (S, )  or  a  Hind  III  site  (S2). 


After  synthesis,  oligonucleotides  were  deprotected  at 
55°C  in  ammonia  overnight;  they  were  then  purified  and 
detritylated  by  reverse-phase  chromatography  (Oligonu- 
cleotide Purification  Cartridges  from  Applied  Biosystems). 
The  oligonucleotides  were  dried  down,  resuspended  in 
sterile  water,  and  concentration  was  estimated  by  absor- 
bance  at  260  nm. 

PCR  amplification 

For  amplification  of  cDNA,  50  pmol  of  each  primer 
and  40%  (20  ^D  of  the  reverse  transcription  reaction  were 
added  directly  to  100  n\  PCR  reactions.  All  other  reaction 
components,  including  Taq  DNA  polymerase,  were  pur- 
chased from  Perkin  Elmer-Cetus  Corp.  (Norwalk,  Con- 
necticut) and  used  as  suggested  by  that  manufacturer.  The 
number  of  amplification  cycles  was  varied  from  25  to  45 
with  the  following  parameters:  denaturation  at  94°C,  1 
min;  annealing  at  37°C,  1  min;  extension  at  72°C.  3  min. 
For  amplification  of  genomic  DNA  using  specific  primers, 
0.3  Mg  Haliolis  rufescens  sperm  DNA.  0.75  ng  Telrahy- 
mena  ihermophila  DNA,  0.5  Mg  Vibrio  harveyi  DNA,  or 
1  Mg  salmon  sperm  DNA  were  added  to  otherwise  identical 
amplification  reactions.  (The  Haliolis.  Telraliymena.  and 
I  'ibrio  genomic  DNA  samples  were  generously  provided 
by  Jay  Groppe.  Jennifer  Ortiz,  and  Richard  Showalter, 
respectively.)  All  DNA  samples  were  tested  for  amplifi- 
cation at  amounts  equal  to  or  greater  than  the  number 
of  genome  equivalents  of  the  Haliolis  DNA.  Because  DNA 
samples  were  in  TE  buffer,  additions  were  adjusted  such 
that  the  total  amount  of  TE  (and  thus  the  concentration 
of  EDTA)  in  all  PCR  reactions  was  equal.  Optimum 
magnesium  concentration,  primer  concentration,  and  cy- 
cling parameters  were  determined  empirically.  For  am- 
plification of  genomic  DNA,  25  pmol  of  each  specific 
primer  was  used;  the  final  concentration  of  magnesium 
was  increased  from  the  standard  1.5  mA/  to  2  mAI:  and 
a  total  of  30  amplification  cycles  was  performed  as  before, 
except  that  the  annealing  temperature  was  raised  to  45°C 
for  the  2nd  cycle  and  to  55°C  for  the  3rd-30th  cycles.  A 
5  min  extension  step  (72°C)  was  added  after  the  last  cycle. 

Gel  elect rophoresis  for  analysis  and  purification  of  PCR 
reaction  products 

PCR  reaction  products  were  analyzed  by  electrophoresis 
on  agarose  gels  (3%  Nuseive  agarose  plus  1%  Seaplaque 
agarose  in  TBE  for  cDNA-PCR  reactions  and  1.5%  aga- 
rose/TAE  buffer  for  genomic  reactions),  run  at  2-6  v/cm. 
and  stained  with  ethidium  bromide.  For  analysis  on  the 
higher  percentage  gels,  samples  were  loaded  into  wells 
cast  from  0.7%  agarose.  One  ^g  of  restriction  enzyme- 
digested  plasmid  (PBR322-BstN  1.  from  New  England 
Biolabs  Inc.,  Beverly,  Massachusetts)  was  included  for 
molecular  weight  markers. 


G«  cDNAs  FROM  LARVAL  CILIA 


321 


For  purification,  amplified  cDNA  was  electrophoresed 
on  3%  low  melting  temperature  agarose  (Mermaid,  from 
Bio  101  Corp.,  La  Jolla),  and  DNA  bands  were  excised 
while  visualized  on  a  365  nm  light  box.  DNA  then  was 
removed  from  the  agarose  by  binding  to  glass  beads  (Glass 
Fog,  from  Bio  101  Corp.).  Genomic  products  were  run 
on  1.5%  agarose  gels  as  described  above;  bands  were  ex- 
cised, and  DNA  was  purified  by  binding  to  glass  beads 
(Geneclean,  from  Bio  101  Corp.). 

DNA  cloning 

Purified  PCR  products  and  a  recombinant  plasmid 
vector  (/7Bluescript  K.SII+,  from  Stratagene  Corp.)  were 
digested  at  37°C  for  1  h  in  20  n\  volumes  with  Hindlll 
followed  by  BamHI  (enzymes  from  New  England  Bio- 
labs).  Digested  products  were  purified  by  gel  electropho- 
resis  as  described  above.  The  purified,  linearized  vector 
( 100  ng)  then  was  ligated  to  an  approximately  equimolar 
amount  of  PCR  product  insert;  this  reaction  was  catalyzed 
by  T4  DNA  ligase  overnight  at  4°C.  Controls  with  no 
added  insert  were  treated  identically.  Transformation  of 
recipient  bacteria  (Epicurian  Coli  XL-1  Blue,  from  Stra- 
tagene Corp.)  was  performed  by  the  method  of  Hanahan 
(1983),  with  modifications  recommended  by  Stratagene 
Corp.  After  transformation,  colonies  with  recombinant 
plasmids  were  identified  on  the  antibiotic-containing  agar 
medium  with  the  chromogenetic  substrate,  X-gal.  Each 
clone  was  then  subcultured  in  5  ml  of  LB  containing  am- 
picillin  and  tetracycline  (37°C,  overnight).  Plasmid  DNA 
was  purified  from  these  cultures  after  lysis  with  alkalai, 
using  the  miniprep  procedure  (Maniatis  el  al.,  1982). 

Cloned  plasmid  DNA  was  digested  as  above  with  BamH 

I  and  Hind  III  simultaneously,  and  separately  with  BssH 

II  (for  which  the  plasmid  has  two  sites,  flanking  the  BamH 
I  and  Hind  III  sites).  Restriction  digests  were  analyzed  by 
agarose  gel  electrophoresis.  Plasmid  DNA  was  purified  by 
centrifugation  chromatography  (Sephacryl  S-400  Mini- 
prep  Spun  Columns,  from  Pharmacia,  Piscataway,  New 
Jersey). 

DNA  sequence  analysis 

Di-deoxy  sequencing  reactions  were  performed  with 
modified  T7  DNA  polymerase  (Sequenase  II;  United 
States  Biochemical,  Cleveland,  Ohio)  and  5'  [«-35S]dATP, 
1 100  Ci/mmol  (Amersham)  by  procedures  modified  from 
Sanger  el  al.  (1977).  Primers  used  for  sequencing  were 
plasmid  primers  T7,  T3,  KS,  and  SK  (purchased  from 
Stratagene)  and  the  specific  primers  S,  and  S:,  described 
above.  Reactions  were  analyzed  by  electrophoresis  on  8% 
polyacrylamide-50%  urea  wedge  sequencing  gels.  Gels 
were  washed  in  10%  acetic  acid- 10%  methanol,  dried  with 
vacuum  at  80°C,  and  subjected  to  autoradiography.  The 
autoradiograms  were  read  with  a  sonic  digitizer,  and  the 


resulting  sequences  analyzed  with  the  aid  of  the  Pustell 
Sequence  Analysis  software  (IBI  Macintosh). 

Results 

PCR  amplification  of  cilia  Ga  cDNA 

Poly  A+  mRNA  was  isolated  from  the  purified  cilia  of 
7-9-day-old,  competent  larvae  of  Haliotis  rufescens.  as 
described  in  the  Methods.  Starting  with  about  10"  larvae, 
typical  yields  were  300-400  mg  (wet  weight)  cilia,  50  ng 
total  RNA,  and  1  ^g  of  poly  A+  mRNA  (  =  2%  of  total 
RNA).  AMV  reverse  transcriptase  was  used  to  catalyze 
random-hexamer  primed  synthesis  of  first  strand  cDNA 
from  the  purified  mRNA,  and  the  resulting  mRNA-cDNA 
duplex  was  then  used  as  template  for  PCR  amplification 
with  the  degenerate  primers  corresponding  to  the  con- 
served G  and  G'  domains,  as  described  in  the  Methods. 

The  primers  used  clearly  directed  the  amplification  of 
a  196  bp  product  from  the  cDNA  templates  prepared  from 
the  larval  cilia  (Fig.  la).  The  size  of  this  product  is  within 
the  range  predicted  for  a  G«  cDNA  domain  lying  between 
the  highly  conserved  G  and  G'  domains.  This  result  sug- 
gests that  the  cilia  purified  from  Haliotis  rufescens  larvae 
may  contain  mRNA  coding  for  a  G«  protein. 

The  positive  result  shown  in  Figure  la  allowed  us  to 
further  optimize  the  primers  used  for  PCR-amplification. 
The  upstream  primer  used  in  the  first  PCR  amplifications 
was  relatively  short,  consisting  of  a  degenerate  pool  of  17- 
mers.  This  primer  only  weakly  amplified  the  positive  con- 
trol template,  cDNA  from  bovine  retina,  a  tissue  highly 
enriched  for  the  G«  known  as  transducin  (results  not 
shown).  The  addition  of  nine  nucleotides  containing  the 
sequence  of  the  BamHI  restriction  endonuclease  site  to 
the  5'-end  of  the  1 7-mers.  to  generate  the  D,  primers  (see 
Materials  and  Methods),  makes  efficient  amplification  of 
the  control  Ga  sequence  from  bovine  retina  cDNA  pos- 
sible (Fig.  Ib).  [Similarly,  we  had  found  earlier  that  ad- 
dition of  the  nine  nucleotides  containing  the  sequence  of 
the  Hind  III  restriction  site  to  the  5'-end  of  short  down- 
stream primers,  to  generate  the  26-mer  D2  and  D,  pools, 
also  significantly  enhanced  the  efficiency  of  these  oligo- 
nucleotides  as  primers.  These  non-matching  nucleotides 
do  not  reduce  primer  specificity.  Similar  observations  have 
been  reported  by  others  (e.g..  Mack  and  Sninsky,  1988).] 
The  results  in  Figure  Ib  show  the  downstream  primers 
in  the  degenerate  pool  D;  are  more  effective  than  those 
in  D3  for  detecting  and  amplifying  Go  cDNA  sequences 
from  both  bovine  retina  and  Haliotis  rufescens  larval  cilia. 
Primer  D,  differs  from  D:  by  two  nucleotides  and  appar- 
ently fails  to  hybridize  efficiently  to  the  target  G  protein 
cDNA  sequence.  Agarose  gel  electrophoresis  of  PCR 
products  amplified  with  the  optimized  26-mer  primers 
(D,  and  D2)  reveals  the  expected  205  bp  product  from 


322 


L.  M.  WODICKA  AND  D.  E.  MORSE 


cDNAs  from  both  the  larval  cilia  and  bovine  retina.  The 
product  is  nine  nucleotides  longer  than  that  seen  in  Figure 
la,  as  expected  because  the  upstream  primer  (D, )  is  nine 
nucleotides  longer  than  that  used  in  the  first  experiment. 
The  cilia  cDNA  required  more  cycles  of  amplification 
(35)  than  did  the  bovine  retina  cDNA  (25)  before  the 
product  on  an  ethidium  bromide  stained  gel  could  be 
visualized.  Thus,  there  may  be  greater  primer-template 
mismatch,  or  the  target  mRNA  may  be  less  abundant,  in 
the  larval  cilia. 

A  control  PCR  reaction  with  no  added  DNA  template, 
a  test  for  DNA  contamination  of  reagents,  yielded  no  de- 
tectable PCR  products  amplified  after  45  cycles  (Fig.  Ib). 
In  addition,  no  amplified  PCR  products  were  observed 
in  the  following  control  reactions  (not  shown):  (a)  no 
primers  in  the  PCR  reaction;  (b)  no  Taq  polymerase  in 
the  PCR  reaction;  and  (c)  no  reverse  transcriptase  in  the 
cDNA  reaction. 

Cloning  and  sequence  analysis  of  Ga  cDNA 

The  205  bp  PCR  product  from  the  cilia  cDNA  was 
cloned  in  the  plasmid  vector  as  described  in  Materials 
and  Methods.  Twelve  transformant  colonies  were  picked 
and  subcultured;  gel  electrophoresis  of  the  restriction  en- 
zyme-digested plasmid  DNA  showed  that  1 1  of  these 
clones  contained  inserts  of  the  correct  size. 

Sequence  analysis  of  three  of  the  cloned  cilia  PCR 
products  revealed  two  unique  cDNA  sequences  (Fig.  2). 
As  shown,  both  of  these  share  a  number  of  the  highly 
conserved  residues  of  other  G«  proteins  in  the  G-G'  re- 
gion. Two  out  of  the  three  clones  proved  to  have  identical 


Cilia 


DNA 


(S)    A.   rRet. 


-Cilia- 


nNone 


196  bp  — 


2nd  Primer  -   (X)  D2  D3   D2   D2   D2  D3  D2   D2 
Cycles  -     30    30    30    30    35    40    40    45    45 


121  — 


9    10 


Figure  l(b).  PCR  amplification  of  larval  cilia  cDNA  using  degenerate 
primers  D,  and  either  D,  or  D,.  Lanes:  ( 1 )  molecular  weight  standards; 
(2)  \  DNA  and  primers  as  PCR  control;  (3)  bovine  retina  cDNA.  primers 
D|  and  D2.  30  cycles;  (4)  bovine  retina  cDNA,  primers  D,  and  D3,  30 
cycles;  (5)  cilia  cDNA,  primers  D,  and  D,,  30  cycles:  (6)  cilia  cDNA, 
primers  D,  and  D,,  35  cycles;  (7)  cilia  cDNA,  primers  D,  and  D2,  40 
cycles;  (8)  cilia  cDNA,  primers  D,  and  D3,  40  cycles  (9)  cilia  cDNA. 
primers  D,  and  D2,  45  cycles;  (10)  no  DNA  template,  primers  D,  and 
D:,  45  cycles. 


nucleotide  (and  deduced  protein)  sequences  over  the  51 
amino  acid  region  between  the  primers.  This  Haliotis  G 
protein  «  subunit  (G«l )  differs  in  the  region  analyzed  by 
only  one  amino  acid  from  the  sequence  of  mouse  brain 
G«  1 1 ,  a  member  of  the  newly  discovered  Gq  class  of  a 
subunits  (Strathmann  el  ai,  1989;  Strathmann  and  Si- 
mon, 1990).  This  sequence  differs  significantly  in  the  re- 
gion analyzed  from  all  other  known  classes  of  G  protein 
a  subunits  (Gs,  G,,  G0n,  G0,  G,,  GJ  from  mammals. 
Drosophila,  and  yeast.  The  second  G  protein  sequence 
from  the  larval  cilia  (Haliotis  G«2)  is  most  homologous 
to  G0  and  GJ  from  Drosophila, 


Figure  l(a).  G  protein  a  subunit  cDNA  from  cilia,  amplified  by 
PCR.  Product  from  50  cycles  of  amplification;  primers  were  a  17-mer 
variant  of  D,  (without  the  BamHI  site)  and  D2.  Number  of  base-pairs 
in  product  is  indicated.  Details  in  Materials  and  Methods. 


Amplification,  cloning,  and  sequence  analysis  oj  Got 
from  Haliotis  genomic  DNA 

The  nucleotide  sequence  from  the  Haliotis  ntfescens 
larval  cilia  Gal  clone  was  used  to  design  specific  (i.e.. 
non-degenerate)  primers  to  amplify  the  corresponding  re- 
gion of  G«  from  H.  rufescens  sperm  genomic  DNA.  The 
specific  primers  (S}  and  S:)  were  based  on  regions  of  the 
Haliotis  sequence  that  differed  significantly  from  other 
G«  protein  sequences  (Fig.  5;  cf.  Fig.  2). 


Ga  cDNAs  FROM  LARVAL  CILIA 


323 


Inlron 


HaliotisGal 
Haliotis  Gtt2 
Mouse  Gq 
Drosoph.  Gj 
Drosoph.  G0 
Rat  G0 
RatGx 
Bov.  Transd. 
Yeast  GP1 
Yeast  GP  2 
Drosoph.  Gs 
RatGolf 


Ident/51 
Gal  Ga2 

26 


ENVTSIMFLVALSEYDQVLVESDSENRMEESKALFRTII  TYPWFQNSSVIL 

EG  VTA  I  1  FI  VAMSEYDLTLAEDQEMNRMMESMKLFDS  ICNMCWFTDTSI  1L  26 

ENVTS1MFLVALSEYDQVLVESDNENRMEESKALFRTI  I  TYPWFQNSSVIL  SO  26 

EGVTAI  1  FCVALSGYDLVLAEDEEMNRMIESLKLFDS  ICNSKWFVETSI  IL  27  41 

EDVTAI  1  FCVAMSEYDQVLHEDETTNRMQESLKLFDS  ICNNKWFTDTSi  1L  28  41 

EDVTAI  I  FCVALSGYDQVLHE0ETTNRMHESLMLFDS  rCNNKFF  I  DTSI  I  L  27  36 

EGVTAI  IFCVELSGYDLKLYE0NQT  SRMAESLRLFDS  ICNNNWFI  NTSLIL  25  33 

EGVTC1  I FI AALSAYDMVLVE0DEVNRMHESLHLFNS ICNHRYFATTSIVL  26  32 

EGITAVLFVLAMSEYDQMLFEDERVNRMHESIMLFDTLLNSKWFKDTPFIL  23  30 

DNVTLV  I  FCVS  LSEYDQTLMEDKNQNR  FQESLVLFDN  I  VNS  RWFARTSVVL  25  27 

NDVTAI  I  FVTACSSYNMVLREDPTQNRLRESLDLFKS  IWNN  RWLR  T  I  S  I  IL  21  27 

NDVTAI  IYVAACSSYNMVI  REDNN T NR L RESLDLF E S IWNNRWLR T I S  I  I  L  18  25 

:•:•:.:•:•:•:•:•:•:•:-:•:  :•:•:•:•:•:•:•:•.•:•.•.•.•.-.•.-. 

Figure  2.  Ga  sequences  from  cilia.  Deduced  amino  acid  sequences  of  the  cloned  PCR  products  (Gal 
and  G«2)  from  Haliolis  rufescens  cilia  cDNA  are  compared  with  the  corresponding  regions  of  other  Ga 
subunits.  Residues  highly  homologous  in  several  Ga  proteins  are  indicated  by  shading.  Region  shown  is 
between  (not  including)  degenerate  primers  D,  and  D;.  The  number  of  amino  acids  identical  to  Haliotis 
Gal  and  Ga2  (of  5 1  total)  is  shown  for  each  G«.  Sequences  used  to  design  specific  primers  S,  and  S2  are 
shown  by  arrows:  position  of  the  intron  in  genomic  DNA  is  indicated.  Standard  (IUPAC)  one-letter  amino 
acid  code  is  used. 


Two  distinct  DNA  products  were  seen  when  Haliotis 
genomic  DNA  was  amplified  by  PCR  reactions  with  the 
S,  and  S:  primers,  and  the  products  were  analyzed  on  an 
agarose  gel  (Fig.  3).  The  specificity  of  these  primers  for 
Haliotis  DNA  is  evident  by  their  failure  to  direct  ampli- 
fication of  genomic  DNA  sequences  from  Tetrahymena, 
Vibrio,  or  salmon  sperm  in  otherwise  identical  PCR  re- 
actions. 

The  1.25  kb  and  1.45  kb  Haliolis  genomic  PCR  prod- 
ucts were  electrophoretically  purified,  separately  amplified 
again,  and  analyzed  electrophoretically  (Fig.  4).  The  suc- 
cessful purification  of  the  two  genomic  PCR  products  is 
shown  by  the  lack  of  visible  contamination  after  this  sec- 
ond round  of  amplification  and  gel  electrophoresis.  Each 
purified  PCR  product  was  cloned  as  described  above,  and 
the  cloned  inserts  were  then  sequenced  using  primers 
matched  to  the  vector. 

During  the  cloning  step  we  discovered  that  the  larger 
of  the  two  genomic  PCR  products  had  an  internal  Hind 
III  site  not  found  in  the  smaller  product.  This  difference 
apparently  resides  in  an  intron,  a  non-coding  sequence 
not  present  in  the  cDNA  (see  below).  Although  the  re- 
sulting Hind  Ill-Hind  III  fragment  was  not  cloned,  a  par- 
tial sequence  for  this  region  was  obtained  from  the  direct 
sequencing  of  the  uncloned  PCR  products  using  S,  and 


S:  as  primers  for  the  sequencing  reaction.  While  this 
method  of  direct  sequencing  proved  useful  in  this  case, 
the  quality  of  the  sequencing  reactions  was  highly  variable 
(t/McCabe,  1989). 

The  genomic  PCR  product  sequences  were  identical  to 
the  G«l  cDNA  sequence  from  the  cilia,  and  to  one  an- 
other, in  the  putative  coding  regions.  But  both  of  the 
cloned  genomic  sequences  contain  an  intron  that  inter- 
rupts the  coding  sequence.  The  exact  position  of  this  in- 
tron between  exons  6  and  7  is  conserved  in  the  genomic 
sequences  corresponding  to  the  Haliotis  Gal  and  the  G0 
and  G,  of  mammals  and  Drosophila  (Figs.  2,  5).  The  two 
Haliotis  genomic  sequences  prove  to  be  highly  homolo- 
gous to  one  another  at  both  ends  of  the  introns  that  are 
adjacent  to  the  coding  regions  (corresponding  to  exons  6 
and  7  in  other  species;  cf.  Fig.  5);  however,  the  two  introns 
differ  in  length  by  about  200  bp. 

Discussion 

The  larvae  of  Haliotis  rufescens  provide  a  uniquely 
tractable  model  system  for  resolving  and  analyzing  the 
chemosensory  receptors  and  signal  transducers,  and  the 
mechanisms  of  their  functional  integration,  controlling 
behavior  and  development  in  response  to  chemical  signals 


324 


L.  M.  WODICKA  AND  D.  E.  MORSE 


bp 


1857  — 

1450  — 

1250  — 

1060  — 

929  — 


7 


Figure  3.  PCR  amplification  of  genomic  DNA  using  Haliotis-sped&c 
pnmers  S,  and  S:  (30  cycles).  Lanes:  1 1 )  molecular  weight  standards;  (2) 
and  (3)  0.3  Mg  Haliolis  sperm  DNA;  (4)  0.75  ^g  Tetrahymena  DNA;  (5) 
0.5  Mg  Vibrio  DNA;  (6)  1  Mg  salmon  sperm  DNA;  (7)  no  DNA.  Other 
details  in  Materials  and  Methods. 


from  the  environment  (Morse,  1990a,  b).  Purification  of 
cilia  in  milligram  quantities  from  the  cultured  larvae  has 
allowed  us  to  analyze  the  chemosensory  receptors  and 
signal  transducers  ///  vitro  (Baxter,  1991;  Baxter  and 
Morse,  in  prep.),  and  (as  shown  here)  to  isolate  mRNAs 
encoding  some  of  these  elements  and  conduct  analyses  at 
the  cDNA  sequence  level.  We  have  shown  here  that  cilia 
purified  from  //.  rufescens  larvae  contain  polyadenylated 
mRNA,  and  that  this  mRNA  includes  sequences  corre- 
sponding to  two  Ga  signal  transducing  proteins. 

The  central  role  of  the  G  proteins  as  chemosensory 
signal  transducers  was  confirmed  by  ///  vitro  studies  of 
olfactory  cilia  isolated  from  frog  (Pace  et  ai,  1985).  Jones 
and  Reed  (1989)  recently  have  identified  a  unique  G0|f 
from  the  ciliated  sensory  epithelium  of  the  rat  olfactory 
mucosa;  the  sequence  of  this  G  protein  a  subunit  was 
determined  by  analysis  of  its  cDNA.  G  protein  also  acts 
as  the  primary  chemosensory  signal  transducer  in  taste 
receptor  cells  in  the  frog;  this  G  protein  controls  the  ac- 
tivation of  adenyl  cylase,  with  the  resulting  sequential  ac- 
tivation of  a  protein  kinase  and  membrane  depolarization 
(Avenet  et  ai,  1988).  G  protein  transduction  of  chemo- 
sensory stimulation  in  catfish  controls  an  inositol  tri- 
phosphate  (protein  kinase)  cascade  (Huque  et  ai.  1987). 

Although  distal  localization  of  mRNA  has  been  ob- 
served in  other  systems  (Merlie  and  Sanes,  1985;  Garner 
et  ai.  1988;  Kosik  et  ai.  1989),  the  results  reported  here 
are  the  first  of  which  we  are  aware  in  which  mRNA  has 
been  purified,  and  the  corresponding  cDNA  synthesized, 
amplified,  cloned,  and  sequenced  from  purified  cilia.  Be- 


bp 


1857  — 

1450  — 

1250  — 

1060  — 

929  — 


Figure  4.  Haliolia  genomic  PCR  products  after  gel  purification  and 
30  additional  cycles  of  amplification  with  specific  primers  S,  and  Si. 
Lanes:  (I)  molecular  weight  standards;  (2)  ca.  1.45  kb  genomic  PCR 
product;  (3)  ca.  1.25  kb  genomic  PCR  product;  (4)  molecular  weight 
standards. 


cause  the  mRNA  from  which  the  G  protein  sequences 
were  identified  was  extracted  from  cilia  that  had  been 
purified  and  washed  by  four  sequential  cycles  of  differ- 
ential centrifugation,  it  was  probably  not  contaminated 
by  free  RNA  released  from  the  larvae.  Electron  micros- 
copy shows  no  other  cell  fragments  or  cells  contaminating 
the  purified  cilia  (Baxter,  1991;  Baxter  and  Morse,  in 
prep.).  //;  situ  hybridization  will  be  required,  however,  to 
verify  the  intraciliary  localization  of  the  Get  mRNA.  Three 
independent  lines  of  evidence  confirm  that  the  source  of 


P 

1          2        3 

G1 

4567            8 

G                R 

9        10        11 

1        1         1 

III              1 

III                1 

1         2        3 

456 

7                8 

G,a/Goa    I          III  III  I 


HaliotisGa 
PCR  Product 


S2          D2 


Figure  5.  Genomic  organization  of  mammalian  G  protein  «  subunits 
(after  Kaziro  ef  a/.,  1  990).  Exons  (coding  regions)  are  numbered;  vertical 
lines  indicate  introns.  Conserved  domains  P  (GTP  hydrolysis),  G'  and 
G  (GTP  binding)  and  R  are  shown  above.  [In  the  convention  of  Halliday 
(  1  984).  domain  G  also  is  designated  G,  but  the  other  domains  are  des- 
ignated differently.].  The  region  of  the  Haliotis  G  protein  corresponding 
to  mammalian  G0  and  G,  exons  6  and  7  is  expanded  to  show  relative 
positions  and  orientations  of  PCR  primers.  D,,  D^.and  D,  are  degenerate 
primers  used  for  cDNA  amplification;  S,  and  S:  are  the  Haliotis-specific 
pnmers  used  lor  genomic  amplification. 


G«  cDNAs  FROM  LARVAL  CILIA 


325 


the  G  protein  sequences  characterized  could  not  have 
come  from  bacterial  contamination:  (1)  the  mRNA  se- 
lected was  poly  A+;  (2)  the  sequence  of  Gal  determined 
corresponds  exactly  to  a  sequence  in  the  genomic  DNA 
(from  sperm)  of  Haliotis  rufescens:  and  (3)  that  genomic 
sequence  contains  an  intron  (at  the  expected  position) 
that  would  be  lacking  from  bacterial  DNA.  That  these 
sequences  were  from  Haliotis  mRNA,  and  not  from  pos- 
sible contaminants,  was  further  confirmed  by  the  failure 
of  the  unique  ciliary  sequences  to  direct  amplification  of 
any  homologous  sequences  in  control  samples  of  bacterial, 
protozoan,  or  fish  DNA,  under  conditions  in  which  the 
perfectly  homologous  sequences  were  detected  and  am- 
plified from  Haliotis  genomic  (sperm)  DNA. 

The  cDNA  sequences  that  we  obtained  from  mRNA 
purified  from  the  cilia  isolated  from  Haliotis  rufescens 
larvae  reveal  two  apparent  G  protein  «  subunit  sequences. 
Although  definitive  characterization  must  await  comple- 
tion of  the  entire  translated  sequences,  our  identification 
of  these  sequences  as  members  of  the  G  protein  family  is 
strengthened  by  the  finding  that  Gal  is  virtually  identical 
(50/51  residues)  to  Gqa  in  the  region  sequenced.  and  the 
observation  that  the  most  highly  conserved  amino  acids 
in  this  region  of  the  other  known  Ga  proteins  also  are 
conserved  in  the  Gal  and  Ga2  sequences  from  the  Hal- 
iotis larvae  (Fig.  2).  The  genomic  sequences  that  we  thus 
far  have  characterized  correspond  exactly  to  their  cDNAs, 
and  contain  the  intron  at  the  same  position  between  the 
G  and  G'  domains  as  those  found  in  mammalian  G  pro- 
tein genes. 

The  two  Ga  sequences  obtained  from  the  cilia  of  Hal- 
iotis larvae  are  clearly  related  to  the  Go  sequences  from 
other  species  (Fig.  2)  (and  unrelated  to  tubulin,  for  ex- 
ample). Yet  the  two  sequences  differ  significantly  from 
one  another.  The  larval  cilia  Gal  sequence  is  highly  ho- 
mologous to  that  of  the  corresponding  domain  of  the  alpha 
subunit  of  the  mammalian  Gq.  This  is  of  particular  in- 
terest, in  light  of  the  finding  that  Gq  is  a  pertussis  toxin- 
insensitive  regulator  of  phospholipase  C  (Strathmann  and 
Simon,  1990;  Smrcka  et  al,  1991),  and  our  observation 
that  the  lysine-dependent  regulatory  pathway  in  Haliotis 
larvae  is  mediated  by  a  pertussis  toxin-insensitive  G  pro- 
tein-phospholipase  C-protein  kinase  cascade  (Baxter  and 
Morse,  1987;  Baxter,  1991;  Baxter  and  Morse,  in  prep.). 
Gal  is  markedly  different  from  G,,  Gs,  G0,  Gx,  and  G0ir 
characterized  from  other  systems,  whereas  the  larval  cilia 
Go2  sequence  is  significantly  more  closely  related  to  G, 
and  G0  (from  Drosophila  and  rat)  than  it  is  to  the  Gs  and 
G0|f  from  these  species.  We  are  now  in  the  process  of  iden- 
tifying the  Ga  sequence  corresponding  to  the  transducing 
protein  specifically  activated  by  the  lysine  receptors  in  the 
isolated  cilia.  These  experiments  are  facilitated  by  the  ob- 
servation that  lysine  binding  to  the  ciliary  receptors  ac- 
tivates the  associated  Ga  protein,  increasing  its  radioactive 


labeling  with  ADP-ribose  catalyzed  by  cholera  toxin 
(Baxter  and  Morse,  in  prep.). 

In  molluscan  larvae,  the  cilia  of  the  cephalic  apical  tuft 
and  of  the  propodium  have  been  suggested  to  mediate 
chemosensory  substratum-recognition  and  the  resulting 
control  of  metamorphosis  (Raven,  1958;  Fretter  and  Gra- 
ham, 1962;Bonar,  1978a,  b;  Chiaand  Koss,  1984;  Yool, 
1985).  Ciliary  receptors  have  been  implicated  in  this  pro- 
cess in  other  invertebrate  larvae  as  well  (e.g.,  Laverack, 
1968;  Carthy  and  Newell,  1968;  Chia  and  Spaulding, 
1972;  Siebert,  1974;  Zimmer  and  Wollacott,  1977;  Eck- 
elbarger,  1978;  Reed  and  Cloney,  1982;  Reed,  1987).  The 
ciliated  cells  of  the  apical  tufts  of  Haliotis  larvae  are  stellate 
neurons  that  appear  anatomically  to  be  primary  chemo- 
sensory receptor  cells  (Yool,  1985).  These  neurons  project 
axons  to  the  cephalic  ganglia;  they  also  send  lateral  den- 
drites  to  a  pair  of  adjacent  mucus  gland  cells  that  are 
stimulated  to  secrete  their  contents  in  one  of  the  first  cel- 
lular changes  observed  in  metamorphosis  (Morse  et  al., 
1980a;  Yool.  1985).  These  neurons  and  their  cilia  dis- 
appear from  the  organism  after  induction  of  metamor- 
phosis; the  time  of  this  loss  coincides  with  the  time  of  loss 
of  the  labeled  morphogenic  receptors  (Trapido-Rosenthal 
and  Morse,  1986a),  supporting  the  suggestion  that  some 
of  the  biochemically  labeled  chemosensory  receptors 
controlling  metamorphosis  may  be  located  on  these  cells 
or  their  cilia. 

Our  finding  that  sufficient  mRNA  can  be  obtained  from 
the  cilia  of  Haliotis  larvae  to  establish  a  cDNA  library 
opens  this  system  to  analyses,  similar  to  that  reported 
here,  of  the  cDNAs  for  the  receptors  and  other  signal 
transducers  of  the  morphogenetic  and  amplifier  pathways 
that  control  metamorphosis.  These  analyses  should  pro- 
vide insights  into  the  mechanisms  of  action,  functional 
integration,  and  evolution  of  the  chemoreceptors  and  their 
associated  signal-transducers  in  the  molluscan  larvae. 

Acknowledgments 

This  research  was  supported  by  grants  from  the  Na- 
tional Science  Foundation  (DCB-87- 18224),  the  Office  of 
Naval  Research  Molecular  Biology  Program  (NOOO 14-87- 
K-0762),  and  the  University  of  California  at  Santa  Barbara 
Training  Grant  in  Marine  Biotechnology.  We  especially 
appreciate  the  expert  advice,  and  time  and  effort  provided 
by  Jay  C.  Groppe.  We  also  gratefully  acknowledge  the 
helpful  suggestions  provided  by  Mel  Simon,  Thomas  T. 
Amatruda,  and  John  Sninsky;  gifts  of  genomic  DNA 
samples  provided  by  Richard  Showalter,  Jennifer  Ortiz, 
and  Jay  Groppe;  and  expert  instruction  and  assistance  in 
the  dissection  of  retinas,  provided  by  Page  Erickson. 

Literature  Cited 

Anholt,  R.  R.  H.  1987.     Primary  events  in  olfaction.  Trends  Biochem. 
Sd.  12:  58-62. 


326 


L.  M.  WODICK.A  AND  D.  E.  MORSE 


Anholt,  R.  R.  H.,  S.  M.  Mumby,  D.  A.  Stoffers,  P.  R.  Girard,  J.  F.  Kuo, 

and  S.  H.  Snyder.  1987.     Transduction  proteins  of  olfactory  receptor 

cells:  identification  of  guanine  nucleotide  binding  proteins  and  protein 

kinase  C.  Biochemistry  26:  788-795. 
Avenet,  P.,  F.  Mofmann,  and  B.  Lindemann.  1988.     Transduction  in 

taste  receptor  cells  requires  cAMP-dependent  protein  kinase.  Nature 

331:  351-354. 
Aviv,  H.,  and  P.  Leder.  1972.     Purification  of  biologically  active  globin 

messenger  RNA  by  chromatography  on  oligothymidylic  acid-cellu- 
lose. Proc.  Nat.  Acad.  Sd.  USA  69:  1408. 
Baloun,  A.  J.,  and  D.  E.  Morse.  1984.     Ionic  control  of  settlement  and 

metamorphosis  in  larval  Haliotis  rufescens  (gastropoda).  Dial.  Bull. 

167:  124-138. 
Baxter,  G.  1991.     Chemosensory  signal  transduction  in  larvae  of  the 

mollusc,  Haliotis  rujescens.  Doctoral  Dissertation,  University  of  Cal- 
ifornia, Santa  Barbara.  94  pp. 
Baxter,  G.,  and  D.  E.  Morse.  1987.     G  protein  and  diacylglycerol  regulate 

metamorphosis  of  planktonic  molluscan  larvae.  Proc.  Nat.  Acad.  Sci. 

USAM:  1867-1870. 
Bonar,  D.  B.  I978a.     Infrastructure  of  a  cephalic  sensory  organ  in  larvae 

of  the  gastropod  Phestilla  sibogae  (Aeohdacea:  Nudibranchia).  Tissue 

Cell  10:  153-165. 
Bonar,  D.  B.  1978b.     Morphogenesis  at  metamorphosis  in  opistobranch 

molluscs.  Pp.  1 77- 1 96  in  Settlement  and  Metamorphosis  of  Marine 

Invertebrate  Larvae,  F.-S.  Chia  and  M.  E.  Rice,  eds.  Elsevier/North 

Holland  Biomedical  Press.  New  York. 
Carthy,  J.  D.,  and  G.  E.  Newell,  eds.  1968.     Invertebrate  Receptors 

Academic  Press,  London. 
Chen,  'L.,  and  D.  Lancet.  1984.     Membrane  proteins  unique  to  vertebrate 

olfactory  cilia:  candidates  for  sensory  receptor  molecules.  Proc.  Nat. 

Acad.  Sci.  USA  81:  1859-1863. 
Chia,  F.-S.,  and  R.  Koss.  1984.     Fine  structure  of  the  cephalic  sensory 

organ  in  the  larva  of  the  nudibranch  Rtistunga  pulchra  (Mollusca: 

Opistobranchia.  Nudibranchia).  Zoomorphology  104:  131-139. 
Chia,  F.-S..  and  J.  G.  Spaulding.  1972.     Development  and  juvenile 

growth  of  the  sea  anemone,  Tealia  erassicornis.  Bin/.  Bull  142:  206- 

218. 
Chirgwin,  J.  M.,  A.  E.  Przybyla,  R.  J.  MacDonald,  and  \V.  J.  Ruder. 

1979.     Isolation  of  biologically  active  ribonucleic  acid  from  sources 

enriched  in  nbonuclease.  Biochemistry  18:  5294. 
Chomczynski,  P.,  and  N.  Sacchi.  1987.     Single-step  method  of  RNA 

isolation  by  acid  guanidinium  thiocyanate-phenol-chloroform  ex- 
traction. Anal.  Biochem.  162:  156-159. 
Eckelbarger,  K.  J.  1978.     Metamorphosis  and  settlement  in  the  Sabel- 

lariidae.  Pp.  145-164  in  Settlement  and  Metamorphosis  of  Marine 

Invertebrate  Larvae.  F.-S.  Chia  and  M.  E.  Rice.  eds.  Elsevier,  New 

York. 
Fretter,  V.,  and  A.  Graham,  eds.  1962.     British  Prosobranch  Molluscs. 

Ray  Society,  London. 
Garner,  C.  C.,  R.  P.  Tucker,  and  A.  Matus.  1988.     Selective  localization 

of  messenger  RNA  for  cytoskeletal  protein  MAP2  in  dendrites.  Nature 

336:  674-677. 
Groppe,  J.,  and  D.  E.  Morse.  1989.     Molecular  cloning  of  novel  serine 

protease  cDNAs  from  abalone.  Pp.  285-288  in  Current  Topics  in 

Marine  Biotechnology.  S.  Miyachi,  I.  Karube,  and  Y.  Ishida,  eds. 

Japan  Soc.  Mar.  Biotechnol.,  Tokyo. 
Halliday,  K.  R.  1984.     Regional  homology  in  GTP-binding  proto-on- 

cogene  products  and  elongation  factors.  J.  Cyclic  Nucleolide  Res.  9: 

435_44g. 

Han,  J.  H.,  C.  Stratowa,  and  W.  J.  Rutter.  1987.  Isolation  of  full- 
length  putative  rat  lysophospholipase  cDNA  using  improved  methods 
for  mRNA  isolation  and  cDNA  cloning.  Biochemistry  26:  1617- 
1625. 

Hanahan,  D.  1983.  Studies  on  transformation  of  Eschenchia  coli  with 
plasmids.  /  Mol.  Biol.  166:  557-580. 


Huque,     I  .    J.    G.    Brand,    J.    L.    Rabinowitz,    and    D.    L.    Bailey. 

1987.     Phosphohpid  turnover  in  catfish  barbel  (taste)  epithelium  with 

special  reference  to  phosphatidylinositol-4,5-bisphosphate.  Chem. 

Senses  12:  666-667. 
Jones.  D.  T.,  and  R.  R.  Reed.  1989.     G0if:  an  olfactory  neuron-specific 

G  protein  involved  in  odorant  signal  transduction.  Science  244:  790- 

795. 
kaziro  Y.,  H.  Itoh,  and  M.  Nakafuku.  1990.     Organization  of  genes 

coding  for  G-protem  «  subumts  in  higher  and  lower  eukaryotes.  Pp. 

63-76  in  G  Proteins.  R.  lyengar  and  L.  Birnbaumer,  eds.  Academic 

Press,  San  Diego. 

Kosik,  K.  S.,  J.  E.  Crandall,  E.  J.  Mufson,  and  R.  L.  Neve.  1989.  Tau 
//;  situ  hybridization  in  normal  and  Alzheimer  brain:  localization  in 
the  somatodenditic  compartment.  Ann.  Neurol.  26:  352-361. 

Lancet,  D.,  and  V.  Pace.  1987.  The  molecular  basis  of  odor  recognition. 
Trends  Biochem  Sci.  12:  63-66. 

Laverack,  M.  S.  1968.  On  superficial  receptors.  Symp.  Zoo/.  Sac.  Land. 
23:  299-326. 

Linck,  R.  VV.  1973.  Comparative  isolation  Of  cilia  and  flagella  from 
the  lamellibranch  mollusc,  Aequipecten  irradians.  J.  Cell  Sci.  12: 
345-367. 

Lochrie,  M.  A.,  and  M.  J.  Simon.  1988.  G  protein  multiplicity  in  eu- 
karyotic  signal  transduction  systems.  Biochemistry  27:  4957-4965. 

Mack,  D.  H.,  and  J.  J.  Sninsky.  1988.  A  sensitive  method  for  iden- 
tification of  uncharacterized  viruses  related  to  known  virus  groups: 
hepadnavirus  model  system.  Proc.  Nat.  Acad.  Sci.  USA  85(  18):  6977- 
6981. 

Maniatis,  T.,  E.  F.  Fritsch,  and  J.  Sambrook.  1982.  Molecular  Cloning: 
A  Laboratory  M  annul .  Cold  Spring  Harbor  Laboratory.  Cold  Spring 
Harbor,  New  York. 

McCabe,  P.  C.  1989.  Asymmetric  polymerase  chain  reaction.  Ampli- 
fications 3:  1-3. 

Merlie,  J.  P.,  and  J.  R.  Sanes.  1985.  Concentrations  of  acetylcholine 
receptor  mRNA  in  synaptic  regions  of  adult  muscle  fibres.  Nature. 
317:  66-68. 

Morse,  A.  N.  C,  C.  Froyd,  and  D.  E.  Morse.  1984.  Molecules  from 
cyanobacteria  and  red  algae  that  induce  larval  settlement  and  meta- 
morphosis in  the  mollusc  Haliotis  rufescens.  Mar.  Bio/  81:  293-298. 

Morse,  D.  E.  1985.  Neurotransmitter-mimetic  inducers  of  settlement 
and  metamorphosis  of  marine  planktonic  larvae.  Bull.  Mar.  Sci.  37: 
697-706. 

Morse,  D.  E.  1990a.  Recent  progress  in  larval  settlement  and  meta- 
morphosis: closing  the  gaps  between  molecular  biology  and  ecology. 
Bull.  Mar.  Sci.  46:  465-483. 

Morse,  D.  E.  1990b.  Molecular  mechanisms  controlling  metamorphosis 
in  abalone  larvae.  In  Abalone.  M.  Tegner  and  S.  Shepherd,  eds. 
Blackwell  (in  press). 

Morse,  D.  E.,  H.  Duncan,  N.  Hooker,  and  A.  Morse.  1977.  Hydrogen 
peroxide  induces  spawning  in  molluscs,  with  activation  of  prosta- 
glandin  endoperoxide  synthetase.  Science  196:  298-300. 

Morse,  D.  E.,  N.  Hooker,  H.  Dunan,  and  L.  Jensen.  1979.  -y-amino- 
butyric  acid,  a  neurotransmitter.  induces  planktonic  abalone  larvae 
to  settle  and  begin  metamorphosis.  Science  204:  407-410. 

Morse,  D.  E.,  H.  Duncan.  N.  Hooker,  A.  Baloun,  and  G.  Young. 
1980.  GABA  induces  behavioral  and  developmental  metamorphosis 
in  planktonic  molluscan  larvae.  Fed.  Proc.  39:  3237-3241. 

Pace,  U.,  and  D.  Lancet.  1986.  Olfactory  GTP-binding  protein:  signal- 
transducing  polypeptide  of  vertebrate  chemosensory  neurons.  Proc. 
Nat.  Acad.  Sci.  USA  83:  4947-4951. 

Pace,  II.,  E.  Hanski,  Y.  Saloman.  and  D.  Lancet.  1985.  Odorant-sen- 
sitive  adenylate  cyclase  may  mediate  olfactory  reception.  Nature  316: 
255-258. 


G«  cDNAs  FROM  LARVAL  CILIA 


327 


Raven,  C.  P.  1958.  Morphogenesis:  the  Analysis  ofMolluscan  Devel- 
opment Pergamon  Press,  New  York. 

Reed,  C.  G.  1987.  The  organization  and  isolation  of  the  ciliary  loco- 
motory  and  sensory  organs  of  marine  bryozoan  larvae.  Pp.  397-408 
in  Marine  Biodeterioration,  R.  Turner  and  R.  Nagahhushanam,  eds., 
Oxford  Press.  New  Delhi. 

Reed,  C.,  and  R.  A.  Cloney.  1982.  The  larval  morphology  of  the  marine 
bryozoan  Bowerbankia  graci/is  (Ctenostomata:  Vosiculariodea). 
Zoomorpliiilogy  100:  23-54. 

Rhein,  L.  D.,  and  R.  H.  Cagan.  1980.  Biochemical  studies  of  olfaction: 
isolation,  characterization,  and  odorant  binding  activity  of  isolated 
cilia  from  rainbow  trout  olfactory  rosettes.  Prnc.  Nat.  Aaul.  Sci.  USA 
77:  4412-4416. 

Sanger,  F.,  S.  Nicklen,  and  A.  R.  Coulson.  1977.  DNA  sequencing 
with  chain-terminating  inhibitors.  Pritc.  Nat  Acad.  Sci.  USA  74: 
5463-5467. 

Siebert,  A.  E.  1974.  A  description  of  the  embryology,  larval  develop- 
ment and  feeding  of  the  sea  anemones  Anthopleura  elgantissima  and 
A.  xanthogrammica.  Can.  J.  Zoo/.  52:  1383-1388. 

Smrcka,  V.,  J.  R.  Hepler,  K.  O.  Brown,  and  P.  C.  Sternweis. 
1991.  Regulation  of  polyphosphoinositide-specific  phospholipase 
C  activity  by  purified  Gq.  Science  251:  804-807. 

Strathmann,  M.,  and  M.  I.  Simon.  1990.  G  protein  diversity:  a  distinct 
class  of  «  subunits  is  present  in  vertebrates  and  invertebrates.  Proc 
Nat.  Acad.  Sci.  USA  87:  91 13-9117. 


Stralhmann,  M.,  T.  M.  Wilkie,  and  M.  1.  Simon.  1989.  Diversity  of 
the  G-protein  family:  sequences  from  five  additional  «  subunits  in 
the  mouse.  Proc  Nat.  Acad.  Sci.  USA  86:  7407-7409. 

Trapido-Rosenthal,  H.  G.,  and  D.  E.  Morse.  1985.  L-«,  w-Diamino 
acids  facilitate  induction  of  settlement  and  metamorphosis  in  larvae 
of  a  gastropod  mottusc(Haliotis  mfescens).  J  Comp.  Physiol.  B  155: 
403-414. 

Trapido-Rosenthal,  H.  G.,  and  D.  E.  Morse.  1986a.  Availability  of 
chemosensory  receptors  is  down-regulated  by  habituation  of  larve 
to  a  morphogenetic  signal.  Prtu:  Nat.  Acad.  Sci.  USA  83:  7658- 
7662. 

Trapido-Rosenthal,  11.  G.,  and  D.  E.  Morse.  1986b.  Regulation  of 
receptor-mediated  settlement  and  metamorphosis  in  larvae  of 
a  gastropod  mollusc  (llaliolis  mfescens).  Bull.  Mar.  Sci.  39: 
383-392. 

Watson,  M.  R.,  and  J.  M.  Hopkins.  1962.  Isolated  cilia  from  Tetra- 
hymena  pyriformis.  E\p.  Cell.  Res.  28:  280-295. 

Yool,  A.  J.  1985.  Physiological  analysis  of  the  induction  of  metamor- 
phosis in  larvae  of  Haiiolis  rnlesccnx  (Gastropoda:  Mollusca).  Ph.D. 
Thesis.  University  of  California.  Santa  Barbara.  Ill  pp. 

Zimmer,  R.  L.,  and  R.  M.  \\oolacott.  1977.  Metamorphosis,  ances- 
trulae.  and  coloniality  in  bryozoan  life  cycles.  Pp.  91-142  in  Biology 
of  Brvoioans.  R.  M.  Woolacott  and  R.  L.  Zimmer,  eds..  Academic 
Press,  New  York. 


CONTENTS 


Hanlon,  Roger  T. 

Integrative  neurobiology  and  behavior  of  mollusks 
symposium:  introduction,  perspectives,  and  round- 
table  discussion  197 

Young,  J.  Z. 

Computation  in  the  learning  system  of  cephalopods 
200 

Gilly,  W.  F.,  Bruce  Hopkins,  and  G.  O.  Mackie 
Development  of  giant  motor  axons  and  neural  con- 
trol  of  escape   responses   in   squid   embryos  and 
hatchlings     209 

Williamson,  Roddy 

Factors  affecting  the  sensory  response  characteristics 
of  the  cephalopod  statocyst  and  their  relevance  in 
predicting  swimming  performance  221 

Satterlie,  Richard  A. 

Neural  control  of  speed  changes  in  an  opisthobranch 
locomotory  system  228 

Gillette,  Rhanor 

On  the  significance  of  neuronal  giantism  in  gastro- 
pods    234 

Walters,  Edgar  T. 

A  functional,  cellular,  and  evolutionary  model  of 
nociceptive  plasticity  in  Aplysia 241 

Cleary,  L.  J.,  D.  A.  Baxter,  F.  Nazif,  andj.  H.  Byrne 
Neural  mechanisms  underlying  sensitization  of  a 
defensive  reflex  in  Aphsia  252 


Kiip  term  a  M  ii,  Irving,  Thomas  Teyke,  Steven  C.  Ro- 
sen, and  Klaudiusz  R.  Weiss 

Studies  of  behavioral  state  in  Apl\sia    262 

Alevizos,  A.,  M.  Skelton,  K.  R.  Weiss,  and  J.  Koester 

A  comparison  of  bursting  neurons  in  Aphsia    ....      269 

Ram,  Jeffrey  L.,  Feng  Zhang,  and  Li-Xin  Liu 

Contraction,  serotonin-elicited  modulation,  and 
membrane  currents  of  dissociated  fibers  of  Af)l\iia 
buccal  muscle 276 

Jacklet,  Jon  W. 

Photoresponsiveness  of  Apl\sia  eye  is  modulated  by 

the  ocular  circadian  pacemaker  and  serotonin  .  .  .      284 

Prior,  David  J. 

Control  of  central  and  peripheral  targets  by  a  mul- 
tifunctional peptidergic  interneuron  295 

Kavaliers,  Martin,  and  Klaus-Peter  Ossenkopp 
Opiod  systems  and  magnetic  field  effects  in  the  land 
snail,  Cepaea  nemoralis    301 

Pires,  Anthony,  and  Michael  G.  Hadfield 

Oxidative  breakdown  products  of  catecholamines 
and  hydrogen  peroxide  induce  partial  metamor- 
phosis in  the  nudibranch  Phestilta  sibogae  Bergh 
(Gastropoda:  Opisthobranchia)  310 

Wodicka,  Lisa  M.,  and  Daniel  E.  Morse 

cDNA  sequences  reveal  mRNAs  for  two  Ga  signal 
transducing  proteins  from  larval  cilia 318 


Volume  180 


THE 


Number  3 


BIOLOGICAL 
BULLETIN 


i 


Marine 


JUN 


JUNE,  1991 


Published  by  the  Marine  Biological  Laboratory 


THE 

BIOLOGICAL  BULLETIN 


PUBLISHED  BY 
THE  MARINE  BIOLOGICAL  LABORATOR 


Associate  Editors 


yMarine  Biological 
LIBRARY 

JUN  1  7  1991 


Woods  Hole,  Mass. 


PETER  A.  V.  ANDERSON,  The  Whitney  Laboratory,  University  of  Florida 

DAVID  EPEL,  Hopkins  Marine  Station,  Stanford  University 

J.  MALCOLM  SHICK.  University  of  Maine,  Orono 

Editorial  Board 


GEORGE  J.  AUGUSTINE,  University  of  Southern 

California 


Louis  LEIBOVITZ.  Manne  Biological  Laboratory 


RUDOLF  A.  RAFF,  Indiana  University 
KENSAL  VAN  HOLDE,  Oregon  State  University 
STEVEN  VOGEL,  Duke  University 


Editor:  MICHAEL  J.  GREENBERG,  The  Whitney  Laboratory,  University  of  Honda 
Managing  Editor:  PAMELA  L.  CLAPP,  Marine  Biological  Laboratory 


JUNE,  1991 


Printed  and  Issued  by 
LANCASTER  PRESS,  Inc. 

PRINCE  &  LEMON  STS. 
LANCASTER,  PA 


THE  BIOLOGICAL  BULLETIN 


THE  BIOLOGICAL  BULLETIN  is  published  six  times  a  year  by  the  Marine  Biological  Laboratory,  MBL 
Street,  Woods  Hole.  Massachusetts  02543. 

Subscriptions  and  similar  matter  should  be  addressed  to  Subscription  Manager.  THE  BIOLOGICAL  BUL- 
LETIN. Marine  Biological  Laboratory.  Woods  Hole,  Massachusetts  02543.  Single  numbers.  $25.00.  Sub- 
scription per  volume  (three  issues),  $72.50  ($145.00  per  year  for  six  issues). 

Communications  relative  to  manuscripts  should  be  sent  to  Michael  J.  Greenberg.  Editor-in-Chief,  or 
Pamela  L.  Clapp.  Managing  Editor,  at  the  Marine  Biological  Laboratory,  Woods  Hole,  Massachusetts  02543. 
Telephone:  (508)  548-3705,  ext.  428.  FAX:  508-540-6902. 


POSTMASTER:  Send  address  changes  to  THE  BIOLOGICAL  BULLETIN.  Marine  Biological  Laboratory1. 

Woods  Hole,  MA  02543. 

Copyright  ©  1991,  by  the  Marine  Biological  Laboratory 

Second-class  postage  paid  at  Woods  Hole.  MA,  and  additional  mailing  offices. 

ISSN  0006-3 185 


INSTRUCTIONS  TO  AUTHORS 


The  Biological  Bulletin  accepts  outstanding  original  research 
reports  of  general  interest  to  biologists  throughout  the  world. 
Papers  are  usually  of  intermediate  length  (10-40  manuscript 
pages).  A  limited  number  of  solicited  review  papers  may  be  ac- 
cepted after  formal  review.  A  paper  will  usually  appear  within 
four  months  after  its  acceptance. 

Very  short,  especially  topical  papers  (less  than  9  manuscript 
pages  including  tables,  figures,  and  bibliography)  will  be  pub- 
lished in  a  separate  section  entitled  "Research  Notes."  A  Re- 
search Note  in  The  Biological  Bulletin  follows  the  format  of 
similar  notes  in  Nature.  It  should  open  with  a  summary  para- 
graph of  150  to  200  words  comprising  the  introduction  and  the 
conclusions.  The  rest  of  the  text  should  continue  on  without 
subheadings,  and  there  should  be  no  more  than  30  references. 
References  should  be  referred  to  in  the  text  by  number,  and 
listed  in  the  Literature  Cited  section  in  the  order  that  they  appear 
in  the  text.  Unlike  references  in  Nature,  references  in  the  Re- 
search Notes  section  should  conform  in  punctuation  and  ar- 
rangement to  the  style  of  recent  issues  of  The  Biological  Bulletin. 
Materials  and  Methods  should  be  incorporated  into  appropriate 
figure  legends.  See  the  article  by  Lohmann  et  at.  (October  1990, 
Vol.  179:  214-218)  for  sample  style.  A  Research  Note  will  usually 
appear  within  two  months  after  its  acceptance. 

The  Editorial  Board  requests  that  regular  manuscripts  con- 
form to  the  requirements  set  below;  those  manuscripts  that  do 
not  conform  will  be  returned  to  authors  for  correction  before 
review. 

1 .  Manuscripts.  Manuscripts,  including  figures,  should 
be  submitted  in  triplicate.  (Xerox  copies  of  photographs  are  not 
acceptable  for  review  purposes.)  The  original  manuscript  must 
be  typed  in  no  smaller  than  1 2  pitch,  using  double  spacing  (in- 
cluding figure  legends,  footnotes,  bibliography,  etc.)  on  one  side 
of  16-  or  20-lb.  bond  paper,  81/:  by  1 1  inches.  Please,  no  right 
justification.  Manuscripts  should  be  proofread  carefully  and  er- 
rors corrected  legibly  in  black  ink.  Pages  should  be  numbered 
consecutively.  Margins  on  all  sides  should  be  at  least  1  inch  (2.5 
cm).  Manuscripts  should  conform  to  the  Council  of  Biology  Ed- 
itors Style  Manual.  4th  Edition  (Council  of  Biology  Editors, 
1978)  and  to  American  spelling.  Unusual  abbreviations  should 


be  kept  to  a  minimum  and  should  be  spelled  out  on  first  reference 
as  well  as  defined  in  a  footnote  on  the  title  page.  Manuscripts 
should  be  divided  into  the  following  components:  Title  page. 
Abstract  (of  no  more  than  200  words).  Introduction,  Materials 
and  Methods,  Results,  Discussion,  Acknowledgments,  Literature 
Cited,  Tables,  and  Figure  Legends.  In  addition,  authors  should 
supply  a  list  of  words  and  phrases  under  which  the  article  should 
be  indexed. 

2.  Title  page.     The  title  page  consists  of:  a  condensed  title 
or  running  head  of  no  more  than  35  letters  and  spaces,  the 
manuscript  title,  authors'  names  and  appropriate  addresses,  and 
footnotes  listing  present  addresses,  acknowledgments  or  contri- 
bution numbers,  and  explanation  of  unusual  abbreviations. 

3.  Figures.     The  dimensions  of  the  printed  page.  7  by  9 
inches,  should  be  kept  in  mind  in  preparing  figures  for  publi- 
cation. We  recommend  that  figures  be  about  1 '/:  times  the  linear 
dimensions  of  the  final  printing  desired,  and  that  the  ratio  of 
the  largest  to  the  smallest  letter  or  number  and  of  the  thickest 
to  the  thinnest  line  not  exceed  1:1.5.  Explanatory  matter  gen- 
erally should  be  included  in  legends,  although  axes  should  always 
be  identified  on  the  illustration  itself.  Figures  should  be  prepared 
for  reproduction  as  either  line  cuts  or  halftones.  Figures  to  be 
reproduced  as  line  cuts  should  be  unmounted  glossy  photo- 
graphic reproductions  or  drawn  in  black  ink  on  white  paper, 
good-quality  tracing  cloth  or  plastic,  or  blue-lined  coordinate 
paper.  Those  to  be  reproduced  as  halftones  should  be  mounted 
on  board,  with  both  designating  numbers  or  letters  and  scale 
bars  affixed  directly  to  the  figures.  All  figures  should  be  numbered 
in  consecutive  order,  with  no  distinction  between  text  and  plate 
figures.  The  author's  name  and  an  arrow  indicating  orientation 
should  appear  on  the  reverse  side  of  all  figures. 

4.  Tables,  footnotes,  figure  legends,  etc.     Authors  should 
follow  the  style  in  a  recent  issue  of  The  Biological  Bulletin  in 
preparing  table  headings,  figure  legends,  and  the  like.  Because 
of  the  high  cost  of  setting  tabular  material  in  type,  authors  are 
asked  to  limit  such  material  as  much  as  possible.  Tables,  with 
their  headings  and  footnotes,  should  be  typed  on  separate  sheets, 
numbered  with  consecutive  Roman  numerals,  and  placed  after 


the  Literature  Cited.  Figure  legends  should  contain  enough  in- 
formation to  make  the  figure  intelligible  separate  from  the  text. 
Legends  should  he  typed  double  spaced,  with  consecutive  Arabic 
numbers,  on  a  separate  sheet  at  the  end  of  the  paper.  Footnotes 
should  be  limited  to  authors'  current  addresses,  acknowledg- 
ments or  contribution  numbers,  and  explanation  of  unusual 
abbreviations.  All  such  footnotes  should  appear  on  the  title  page. 
Footnotes  are  not  normally  permitted  in  the  body  of  the  text. 

5.  Literature  cited.  In  the  text,  literature  should  be  cited 
by  the  Harvard  system,  with  papers  by  more  than  two  authors 
cited  as  Jones  ct  til.,  1980.  Personal  communications  and  ma- 
terial in  preparation  or  in  press  should  be  cited  in  the  text  only, 
with  author's  initials  and  institutions,  unless  the  material  has 
been  formally  accepted  and  a  volume  number  can  be  supplied. 
The  list  of  references  following  the  text  should  be  headed  Lit- 
erature Cited,  and  must  be  typed  double  spaced  on  separate 
pages,  conforming  in  punctuation  and  arrangement  to  the  style 
of  recent  issues  of  The  Biological  Bulletin.  Citations  should  in- 
clude complete  titles  and  inclusive  pagination.  Journal  abbre- 
viations should  normally  follow  those  of  the  U.  S.  A.  Standards 
Institute  (USASI),  as  adopted  by  BIOLOGICAL  ABSTRACTS  and 
CHEMICAL  ABSTRACTS,  with  the  minor  differences  set  out  below. 
The  most  generally  useful  list  of  biological  journal  titles  is  that 
published  each  year  by  BIOLOGICAL  ABSTRACTS  (BIOSIS  List  of 
Serials:  the  most  recent  issue).  Foreign  authors,  and  others  who 
are  accustomed  to  using  THE  WORLD  LIST  OF  SCIENTIFIC  PE- 
RIODICALS, may  find  a  booklet  published  by  the  Biological 
Council  of  the  U.K.  (obtainable  from  the  Institute  of  Biology. 
41  Queen's  Gate,  London,  S.W.7,  England,  U.K.)  useful,  since 
it  sets  out  the  WORLD  LIST  abbreviations  for  most  biological 
journals  with  notes  of  the  USASI  abbreviations  where  these  differ. 
CHEMICAL  ABSTRACTS  publishes  quarterly  supplements  of  ad- 
ditional abbreviations.  The  following  points  of  reference  style 
for  THE  BIOLOGICAL  BULLETIN  differ  from  USASI  (or  modified 
WORLD  LIST)  usage: 


A.  Journal  abbreviations,  and  book  titles,  all  underlined 
(for  n al ic ft) 

B.  All  components  of  abbreviations  with  initial  capitals 
(not  as  European  usage  in  WORLD  LIST  e.g.  J.  Cell.  Comp.  Phys- 
io/. NOT/  cell,  comp.  P/ty.tiol.) 

C.  All  abbreviated  components  must  be  followed  by  a  pe- 
riod, whole  word  components  mu.fl  not  (i.e.  J.  Cancer  Re.f.) 

D.  Space  between  all  components  (e.g.  ./.  Cell.  Comp. 
P/n:\io/ .  not  J.Cell.Comp.Physiol.) 

E.  Unusual  words  in  journal  titles  should  be  spelled  out 
in  full,  rather  than  employing  new  abbreviations  invented  by 
the  author.  For  example,  use  Ril  I  'isimhijjelags  Is/endinga  with- 
out abbreviation. 

F.  All  single  word  journal  titles  in  full  (e.g.  I  'eliger.  Ecology, 
Brain). 

G.  The  order  of  abbreviated  components  should  be  the 
same  as  the  word  order  of  the  complete  title  (i.e.  Proc.  and  Trans. 
placed  where  they  appear,  not  transposed  as  in  some  BIOLOGICAL 
ABSTRACTS  listings). 

H.  A  few  well-known  international  journals  in  their  pre- 
ferred forms  rather  than  WORLD  LIST  or  USASI  usage  (e.g.  Na- 
ture. Science.  Evolution  NOT  Nature.  Land.,  Science,  N.Y.: 
Evolution.  Lancaster.  Pa.) 

6.  Reprints,  page  proofs,  and  charges.  A  uthors  receive  their 
first  100  reprints  (without  covers)  free  of  charge.  Additional  re- 
prints may  be  ordered  at  time  of  publication  and  normally  will 
be  delivered  about  two  to  three  months  after  the  issue  date. 
Authors  (or  delegates  for  foreign  authors)  will  receive  page  proofs 
of  articles  shortly  before  publication.  They  will  be  charged  the 
current  cost  of  printers'  time  for  corrections  to  these  (other  than 
corrections  of  printers'  or  editors'  errors).  Other  than  these 
charges  for  authors'  alterations.  The  Biological  Bulletin  does  not 
have  page  charges. 


iii 


CONTENTS 


No.  1.  FEBRUARY  1991 


BEHAVIOR 


De  Vries,  M.  C.,  D.  Rittschof,  and  R.  B.  Forward  Jr. 
Chemical  mediation  of  larval  release  behaviors  in 
the  crab  Neopcmope  \n\i 

Hart,  Michael  W. 

Particle  captures  and  the  method  of  suspension 
feeding  by  echinoderm  larvae    


DEVELOPMENT  AND  REPRODUCTION 


Patterson,  Mark  R. 

The  effects  of  flow  on  polyp-level  prey  capture  in 

an  octocoral,  Alcyonium  nderium    93 

Purcell,  Jennifer  E.,  Frances  P.  Cresswell,  David  G. 

Cargo,  and  Victor  S.  Kennedy 

Differential  ingestion  and  digestion  of  bivalve  larvae 
by  the  scyphozoan  Chiysaora  quinquecirrha  and  the 
ctenophore  A/HCMD'H/MM  li'nl\i 103 

Walters,  Linda  J.,  and  David  S.  Wethey 

Settlement,  refuges,  and  adult  body  form  in  colonial 
marine  invertebrates:  a  field  experiment  112 


Govind,  C.  K.,  Christine  Gee,  and  Joanne  Pearce 

Retarded  and  mosaic  phenotype  in  regenerated  claw 
closer  muscles  of  juvenile  lobsters  28 

Gustafson,  R.  G.,  D.  T.  J.  Littlewood,  and  R.  A.  Lutz 
Gastropod  egg  capsules  and  their  contents  from 
deep-sea  hydrothermal  vent  environments  34 

Longo,  Frank  J.,  and  John  Scarpa 

Expansion  of  the  sperm  nucleus  and  association  of 
the  maternal  and  paternal  genomes  in  fertilized 
Mill/inn  laterals  eggs  56 

Webster,  S.  G.,  and  H.  Dircksen 

Putative  molt-inhibiting  hormone  in  larvae  of  the 
shore  crab  (.'.arcinu*  maenas  L.:  an  immunocyto- 
chemical  approach  65 


ECOLOGY  AND  EVOLUTION 

Carlton,  James  T.,  Geerat  J.  Vermeij,  David  R.  Lind- 
berg,  Debby  A.  Carlton,  and  Elizabeth  C.  Dudley 

The  first  historical  extinction  of  a  marine  inverte- 
brate in  an  ocean  basin:  the  demise  of  the  eelgrass 

limpet  Lottia  alivus 72 

Patterson,  Mark  R. 

Passive  suspension  feeding  by  an  octocoral  in  plank- 
ton patches:  empirical  test  of  a  mathematical  model        8 1 


PHYSIOLOGY 

Bollner,  Tomas,  Jon  Storm-Mathisen,  and  Ole  Petter 
Ottersen 

GABA-like  immunoreactivitv  in  the  nervous  system 

of  Oikopleura  i/imiu  (Appendicularia)    119 

Charmantier,  G.,  and  M.  Charmantier-Daures 

Ontogeny  of  osmoregulation  and  salinity  tolerance 
in  ('./inter  immitm;  elements  of  comparison  with  C. 
burealia  (Crustacea,  Decapoda)  125 

Childress,  J.  J.,  C.  R.  Fisher,  J.  A.  Favuzzi,  R.  E.  Ko- 

chevar,  N.  K.  Sanders,  and  A.  M.  Alayse 

Sulfide-driven  autotrophic  balance  in  the  bacterial 
symbiont-containing  hydrothermal  vent  tubeworm, 
Rift  in  pafh\ptil(i  (ones 135 

Dickson,  John  S.,  Richard  M.  Dillaman,  Robert  D. 

Roer,  and  David  B.  Roye 

Distribution  and  characterization  of  ion  transporting 
and  respiratory  filaments  in  the  gills  of  Procambarus 
eld  ikii  154 

Dobson,  William  E.,  Stephen  E.  Stancyk,  Lee  Ann 

Clements,  and  Richard  M.  Showman 

Nutrient  translocation  during  early  disc  regenera- 
tion in  the  brittlestar  Microphiopholis  gradllima 
(Stimpson)  (Echinodermata:  Ophiuroidea) 167 

McConnaughey,  Ted  A.,  and  Richard  H.  Falk 

Calcium-proton  exchange  during  algal  calcification      1 85 


No.  2,  APRIL  1991 


Hanlon,  Roger  T. 

Integrative  neurobiology  and  behavior  of  mollusks 
symposium:  introduction,  perspectives,  and  round- 
table  discussion  ....  197 


Young,  J.  Z. 

Computation  in  the  learning  system  of  cephalopods 
Gilly,  W.  F.,  Bruce  Hopkins,  and  G.  O.  Mackie 

Development  of  giant  motor  axons  and  neural  con- 


CONTENTS 


trol  of  escape  responses  in  squid  embryos  and 
hatchlings  "2W 

Williamson,  Roddy 

Factors  affecting  the  sensory  response  characteristics 
of  the  cephalopod  statocyst  and  their  relevance  in 
predicting  swimming  performance  221 

Satterlie,  Richard  A. 

Neural  control  of  speed  changes  in  .111  opisthobranch 
locomotory  system  22<S 

Gillette,  Rhanor 

On  the  significance  of  neuronal  giantism  in  gastro- 
pods    234 

Walters,  Edgar  T. 

A  functional,  cellular,  and  evolutionary  model  of 
nociceptive  plasticity  in  Apl\sia 241 

Cleary,  L.  J.,  D.  A.  Baxter,  F.  Nazif,  andj.  H.  Byrne 
Neural  mechanisms  underlying  sensitization  of  a 
defensive  reflex  in  Aplwia  252 

Kupfermann,  Irving,  Thomas  Teyke,  Steven  C.  Ro- 
sen, and  Klaudiusz  R.  Weiss 

Studies  of  behavioral  state  in  Aply.wi    262 

Alevizos,  A.,  M.  Skelton,  K.  R.  Weiss,  and  J.  Koester 


A  comparison  of  "bursting  neurons  in  .\/</vw</    ....      269 

Ram,  Jeffrey  L.,  Feng  Zhang,  and  Li-Xin  Liu 

Contraction,  serotonin-elicited  modulation,  and 
membrane  currents  of  dissociated  fibers  of  Aplysia 
buccal  muscle 276 

Jacklet,  Jon  W. 

Photoresponsiveness  of  Aphsia  eye  is  modulated  bv 

the  ocular  circadian  pacemaker  and  serotonin  .  .  .      284 

Prior,  David  J. 

Control  of  central  and  peripheral  targets  by  a  mul- 
tifunctional peptidergic  interneuron  295 

Kavaliers,  Martin,  and  Klaus-Peter  Ossenkopp 
Opiod  systems  and  magnetic  field  effects  in  the  land 
snail,  Gt'paeii  nemoralis     301 

Pires,  Anthony,  and  Michael  G.  Hadfield 

Oxidative  breakdown  products  of  catecholamines 
and  hydrogen  peroxide  induce  partial  metamor- 
phosis in  the  nudibranch  Pheitillu  \ihagni'  Bergh 
(Gastropoda:  Opisthobranchia)  310 

Wodicka,  Lisa  M.,  and  Daniel  E.  Morse 

cDNA  sequences  reveal  mRNAs  for  two  Got  signal 
transducing  proteins  from  larval  cilia 3  1  IS 


No.  3,  JUNE  1991 


Kravitz,  Edward  A. 

The  rime  of  the  ancient  scientist 


329 


DEVELOPMENT  AND  REPRODUCTION 


Byrne,  M.,  and  M.  F.  Barker 

Embryogenesis  and  larval  development  of  the  as- 
teroid Patiriella  regularis  viewed  by  light  and  scan- 
ning electron  microscopy 332 

Cheng,  Sou-De,  Patricia  S.  Glas,  and  Jeffrey  D.  Green 
Abnormal  sea  urchin  fertilization  envelope  assembly 
in  low  sodium  seawater  346 

Helluy,  S.  M.,  and  B.  S.  Beltz 

Embryonic  development  of  the  American  lobster 
(Homants  americanus):  quantitative  staging  and 
characterization  of  an  embryonic  molt  cycle  ....  355 

Zimmerman,  Kerry  M.,  and  Jan  A.  Pechenik 

How  do  temperature  and  salinity  affect  relative  rates 
of  growth,  morphological  differentiation,  and  time 
to  metamorphic  competence  in  larvae  of  the  marine 
gastropod  Crfpirlula  plana?  372 


ECOLOGY  AND  EVOLUTION 


Alexander,  James  E.,  Jr.,  and  Alan  P.  Covich 

Predation  risk  and  avoidance  behavior  in  two  fresh- 
water snails  ...      387 


Blackstone,  Neil  W.,  and  Leo  W.  Buss 

Shape  variation  in  hydractiniid  hydroids    394 

Miles,  J.  S. 

Inducible  agonistic  structures  in  the  tropical  coral- 
limorpharian,  DI\«>\UIIKI  sanctithomae  406 

Smith,  L.  David,  and  Anson  H.  Mines 

Autotomy  in  blue  crab  (Callmectes  sapidus,  Rathbun) 
populations:  geographic,  temporal,  and  ontogenetic 
variation  416 


ENVIRONMENTAL  PHYSIOLOGY 


Drinkwater,  Laurie  E.,  and  John  H.  Crowe 

Hydration  state,  metabolism,  and  hatching  of  Mono 
Lake  Artemia  cycts    432 


PHYSIOLOGY 


Bowlby,  Mark  R.,  and  James  F.  Case 

Ultrastructure  and  neuronal  control  of  luminous 
cells  in  the  copepod  Ginissia  princepa     440 

Engel,  David  W.,  and  Marius  Brouwer 

Short-term  metallothionein  and  copper  changes  in 
blue  crabs  at  ecdvsis 447 

Gardiner,  David  B.,  Harold  Silverman,  and  Thomas 

H.  Dietz 

Musculature  associated  with  the  water  canals  in 


CONTENTS 


freshwater  mussels  and  response  to  monoamines  in 

I'itrn     453 

Short,  Graham,  and  Sidney  L.  Tamm 

On  the  nature  of  paddle  cilia  and  discocilia 466 

Snyder,  Mark  J.,  and  Ernest  S.  Chang 

Metabolism  and  excretion  of  injected  ['H]-ecdysone 

bv  female  lobsters,  Hnmanif.  iinu'rinniu.^    475 

Takei,  Y.,  A.  Takahashi,  T.  X.  Watanabe,  K.  Naka- 

jima,  S.  Sakakibara,  Y.  Sasayama,  N.  Suzuki,  and  C. 

Oguro 

New  calcitonin  isolated  from  the  ray,  Du\\titi\  nkuji-i     485 

ter  Kuile,  B.  H.,  and  J.  Erez 

Carbon  budgets  for  two  species  of  benthonic  sym- 
biont-bearing  Foraminifera    489 


Weis,  Virginia  M. 

The  induction  of  carbonic  anhvdrase  in  the  sym- 
biotic sea  anemone  Aiptasia  piili'hi'lla    496 


RESEARCH  NOTE 


Ellington,  W.  Ross,  and  Amy  C.  Hines 

Mitochondria!  activities  of  phosphagen  kinases  are 

not  widely  distributed  in  the  invertebrates     505 


Index  to  Volume  180 


r.ns 


Reference:  Biol.  Bull  180:  329-331.  (June.  1991) 


The  Rime  of  the  Ancient  Scientist 

EDWARD  A.  KRAVITZ* 

(With  apologies  to  Coleridge.  Tennyson,  Gray,  Blake,  and  others  too  numerous  to  mention) 


Part  the  First 

//;  which  the  ancient  Scientist  detaineth  and  praiseth  his 
beloved. 

It  was  an  ancient  Scientist, 

And  he  choseth  one  of  three. 

"By  thy  horned  rim  glass  and  balding  pate. 

Now  wherefore  choos't  thou  me? 

Yon  lobster  pot  is  opened  wide 
And  stuffed  full  with  kin: 
Choose  mollusk,  moth  or  fruity  fly, 
Blood-sucking  leech  or  fishy  fin." 

He  holds  him  with  his  skinny  hand, 
"Biochemist  trained  was't  I; 
Thou  art  the  best  of  all  the  rest, 
I'll  give  thee  reasons  why. 

Hail  to  thee  most  noble  of  crustaceans! 
Worthy  of  Homaric  ode, 
Behemoth  of  a  briny  deep. 
Traveler,  of  a  wet,  sandy  road. 


Phalo  h\  Rnben  Huher 


*  The  author  is  the  George  Packer  Berry  Professor  of  Neurobiology  at 
Harvard  Medical  School,  220  Longwood  Avenue,  Boston,  MA  02115. 


No  predator  dare  match  thy  might; 
Who  choose  with  thee  to  fight? 
No  spindly  jaws  or  furry  paws 
Dare  tamper  with  thy  mega-claws. 

Along  the  way,  limbs  that  are  lost 
Can  be  replaced,  no  extra  cost. 
How  practical,  how  devine; 
How  cute  thou  art,  how  truly  fine. 

Inventor  of  contraception!  Thou  scoff?  Thou  doubt? 
What  other  creature  neatly  packs  all  its  sperm  cells  up  in 

sacs? 

Thou  matest  but  once  a  year,  tis  true. 
But  that  one  time  is  quite  a  time! 

If  after  all,  one's  going  to  breed. 
Why  one?  Why  two?  Like  lowly  man. 
Fifty  thousand  is  the  plan. 
Cute  little  larval  brood 
(of  course,  to  mom  they're  mostly  food), 
Which  also  makes  good  sense,  thou  see'st. 
If  mom  didn't  help  by  munching  some. 
The  sea  woulds't  be  a  lobster  slum 
With  millions  and  millions  of  the  beasts; 
No  room  for  fish,  crabs  or  other  feasts. 

Is't  thy  color  really  red? 
Only  when  thou'rt  really  dead. 


This  poem  was  recited  for  the  first  time  on  November  2,  1989,  at  the 
annual  meeting  of  the  Society  for  Neurosciences.  in  Phoenix,  Arizona. 
The  occasion  was  an  Invertebrate  Neurobiology  social  which  had  as  its 
theme:  "Toasts  to  the  Invertebrates."  The  organizer  (M.  J.  Greenberg) 
had  invited  W.  B.  ICristan,  I.  Kupfermann.  J.  G.  Hildebrand.  and  E.  A. 
Kravitz  to  extol  the  advantages,  utility,  and  other  virtues  of  their  favorite 
experimental  animals,  respectively,  the  leech,  the  sea  slug,  the  moth,  and 
the  lobster.  Stirring  words  were  spoken  that  evening,  and  emotions  ran 
high.  Not  one  to  take  a  challenge  lightly,  nor  to  miss  an  opportunity  to 
discuss  the  virtues  of  other  animals  (and  with  a  little  help  from  Alice 
and  a  college  textbook  of  literature)  E.  A.  Kravitz  produced  "The  Rime." 


329 


330 


E.  A.  KRAVITZ 


Greenish  brown  is  more  thy  hue: 

Not  a  bit  like  me  or  you. 

Lobster,  lobster,  cooking  red. 

Boiling  water,  seaweed  bed; 

What  happy  hand  or  eye 

Dare'st  match  drawn  butter,  lemon  juice,  and  lobster  pie? 

Boiled  Aplysia,  tastes  like  rubber; 
Sauteed  leech,  makes  me  shudder! 
Marinated  Manditca.  scales  for  free. 
Did  he  who  cooked  the  lobster,  cook  thee? 

Half  a  leg,  half  a  leg, 

Half  a  leg  onward. 

Back  to  the  lobster  tank  each  time 

Till  8  or  10  appendages  goneward 

Thoughtful  economical  friend! 

Nerve  cells  here,  the  muscles  there, 
The  hormones  all  around; 
Big  cells  thou  art,  and  pluckable. 
Over  and  over  can'st  be  found. 

Transmitters  peptides  and  the  like 
For  studies  fundamental. 

0  joyous  day!  Callooh!  Callay! 
The  data're  transcendental." 

"I  fear  thee,  ancient  Scientist! 

1  fear  thy  skinny  hand." 

"Fear  not!  Fear  not!  Thou  noble  beast! 
List,  while  on  others,  I  expand." 


Part  the  Second 


Fig.  at.  [ 

From  Hemck.  F  H.  1911.  Natural  history  of  the  American  lobster  Bull.  V.  S.  Eur.  Fish.  1909. 
}9: 149-40& 


In  which  the  ancient  Scientist  compareth  lowly  beasties 
to  precious  lobster. 

"First  there's  Aplysia:  sea  hare  'tis  called. 

Sea  hare?  Hare  indeed!  Some  jokester  must  have  been  in 

need 

Of  immortality  for  selecting  the  beast  least  like  the  bunny. 
Who's  really  cute  and  very  funny  (can'st  imagine  Bugs 

Aplysia?). 

Molluscan  mass  of  pulpy  flesh! 
Slimy  inking  shell-less  blob! 
Learner  of  Kandelian  puzzles  that  aren't  hard; 
'Cause  what's  to  train,  in  a  slug  without  a  brain? 
But  I  digress  and  must  continue. 
Flies  can  fly,  tis  true.  So  what? 
It  really  doesn't  mean  a  lot: 
So  can  frisbees,  jets  and  kites: 
Mostly  what  flies  does  is  bites 
They're  things  to  trap,  and  squash,  and  shoo 
With  sticky  paper,  swatters,  and  fancy  kung-fu. 
Their  names  are  inelegant,  crude  and  lewd. 
Like  houseflies,  blowflies,  horseflies  and  gnats; 
I'd  almost  rather  work  with  cats. 
Their  genes  are  cloned,  they  are  well  bred, 
I'll  grant  thee  that.  But  when  that's  said, 
What  good  are  they,  these  beasts  so  small? 
Their  whole  brain  can  set 
In  one  of  thy  glorious  neurons,  pet. 
Guesseth  whom  I  describeth  next! 
Eat  n'eat  n'eat  n'eat  n'eat  n'eat  n'eat  n'eat, 
n'eat  n'eat  n'eat  n'eat  n'eat  n'eat  n'eat  n'eat  n'molt. 
Can'st  yet  tell?  Nay?  Then  I  go  on! 
Eat  n'eat  n'eat  n'eat  n'eat  n'eat  n'eat  n'eat, 
n'eat  n'eat  n'eat  n'eat  n'eat  n'eat  n'eat  n'eat  n'molt. 

Art  bored  yet?  Can'st  bear  more? 
I'll  spare  thee  that! 

Now  twice  or  thrice  more  say  the  same; 
It  doesn't  matter  which  thee  claim. 
For  now  a  change!  O  frabjous  day! 
Can't  hardly  wait!  Can't  barely  stay! 
Eat,  etcetera  and  molt.  And  find  a  hole, 
lie  around,  and  turn  to  goo; 
fly  away  and  live  a  day, 
Make  500  more  like  you. 

Most  interesting, 

Ah,  well. 

They  art  a  lovely  shade  of  green; 

Whilst  dull,  they're  pretty  to  be  seen. 

Leeches,  locusts,  bees  and  ants 


THE  RIME  OF  THE  ANCIENT  SCIENTIST 


331 


Have  their  precious  sycophants. 
Praising  that  ungainly  lot 
of  vampires,  swarms,  and  crop  destroyers. 
And  fat  queens  of  interest  but  to  voyeurs. 

Can'st  not  thou  see  thou  art  the  best? 
Ne'er  another  passeth  test 
Of  beauty,  wit,  charm,  intellect,  and  learning. 
Accepteth  me,  for  thee  I'm  yearning. 

Part  the  Third 

The  noble  beast  agreeeth:  the  ancient  Scientist  getteth 
tenure. 

"I  see  thy  point  old  craggy  beak; 
I  fear  thee  not,  no  more. 
I  am  so  good,  its  understood 
All  others  but  me  foreswore. 

Collect  from  my  nerves; 

Find  all  my  cells; 

Inject  hormones  by  the  score. 

I'll  behave  for  thee,  and  fight,  not  flee. 

To  please  thee  even  more. 

I  warn  thee,  though,  to  leave  me  not. 
Though  funding  turneth  lean. 
For  if  thou  doth  my  chelipeds 
Will  teareth  thee  to  tiny  shreds. 
My  gastric  mill  will  grind  thy  bones 
Till  nothing  doth  remain. 
I'll  chomp!  I'll  chew!  I'll  eat  thee  up! 
Thou'll  never  be  the  same." 

"Fear  not,  fear  not,  my  precious  pet, 
I'll  never  leave  thee  cold. 
I  am  so  happy  I  could  dance. 
If  I  could  be  so  bold." 

In  Xanadu  did  E  A  K 

A  stately  lobster-palace  plan, 

Where  Homar,  with  his  next  of  kin. 


In  burrows  with  two  entrances  in. 
Hides  from  his  nemesis,  man. 

Twilight  tolls  the  knell  of  parting  day. 
The  blowing  winds  wind  slowly  o'er  the  sea. 
The  lobstermen  homeward  plod  their  weary  way, 
And  leave  the  world  to  darkness,  and  to  thee. 


Reference:  Biol.  Bull,  180:  332-345.  (June.  1991) 


Embryogenesis  and  Larval  Development  of  the 

Asteroid  Patiriella  regularis  Viewed  by  Light 

and  Scanning  Electron  Microscopy 


M.  BYRNE1  AND  M.  F.  BARKER 

School  of  Biological  Sciences,  Zoology  A08,  University  of  Sydney,  N.S.W.  2006,  Australia: 
and  Department  of  Zoology,  University  of  Otago,  Dunedin,  New  Zealand 


Abstract.  The  sea  star  Patiriella  regiilaris  (Verrill,  1 867) 
has  indirect  development  through  bipinnaria  and  bra- 
chiolaria  larvae.  Development  of  this  species  is  typical  of 
asteroids  with  planktotrophic  larvae  and  takes  9-10  weeks. 
The  embryos  develop  through  a  wrinkled  blastula  and 
hatch  as  early  gastrulae.  In  contrast  to  most  asteroids,  a 
third  enterocoel  forms  on  the  left  side  of  the  stomach  of 
the  bipinnaria.  This  structure  gives  rise  to  the  left  posterior 
coelom;  its  significance  is  discussed.  We  suggest  that  this 
coelom  is  homologous  to  the  trunk  coelom  in  entero- 
pneust  embryology.  The  surface  features  of  the  larvae  were 
examined  by  scanning  electron  microscopy.  Newly 
hatched  gastrulae  are  covered  by  cilia,  and  the  bipinnaria 
have  bands  of  cilia  that  follow  the  contours  of  the  larval 
processes.  A  previously  undescribed  plug-like  structure 
positioned  on  the  post-oral  surface  appears  to  function  as 
a  seal  for  the  mouth.  Brachiolaria  larvae  have  three  bra- 
chiolar  arms  and  a  centrally  located  adhesive  disc.  Each 
arm  is  covered  by  adhesive  papillae.  Raised  epithelial  cells 
that  dot  the  surface  of  the  papillae  and  adhesive  disc  may 
be  batteries  of  secretory  cells.  The  brachiolar  arms  have 
an  extracellular  coat  that  may  serve  as  a  protective  cover 
for  the  adhesive  surfaces.  Competent  brachiolaria  swim 
along  the  substratum  and  exhibit  searching  behavior  with 
flexure  of  the  median  brachium.  They  settle  on  the  un- 
dersides of  natural  shell  substrata  and  do  not  respond  to 
a  primary  algal  film.  Shade  appears  to  be  an  important 
factor  in  settlement  and  metamorphosis  in  P.  regularis. 
Metamorphosis  takes  5-6  days,  and  the  post-larvae  take 
up  a  free  existence  at  a  diameter  of  450-500  ^m.  The 


Received  20  November  1990;  accepted  8  March  1991. 
1  Present  address:  Department  of  Histology  and  Embryology,  F-13, 
University  of  Sydney,  N.S.W.  2006. 


indirect  development  of  P.  regularis  contrasts  with  the 
lecithotrophic  and  viviparous  modes  of  development  of 
other  Patiriella  species  and  provides  the  comparative  basis 
to  determine  the  ontogenic  changes  involved  with  evo- 
lution of  direct  development  in  the  genus.  The  use  of  the 
divergent  life  histories  of  Patiriella  as  a  model  system  for 
the  study  of  evolutionary  change  in  development  is  dis- 
cussed. 

Introduction 

The  spinulosan  sea  star  Patiriella  regularis  (Verrill, 
1867)  is  common  in  New  Zealand  waters,  ranging  from 
the  intertidal  zone  to  100  m  depth  (Mortensen,  1921; 
Crump,  1971).  This  species  is  a  member  of  the  Patiriella 
group  of  which  there  are  eleven  species  in  the  Australia- 
New  Zealand  region  (Dartnall,  1971;  Keough  and  Dart- 
nail,  1978).  A  remarkable  feature  of  these  asteroids  is  the 
diversity  of  life  histories  that  they  exhibit,  ranging  along 
a  continuum  from  indirect  to  direct  development  (Dart- 
nall, 1971;  Lawson-Kerr  and  Anderson,  1978;  Byrne, 
1991;  Table  I).  P.  regularis  spawns  small  eggs  and  develops 
indirectly  through  planktotrophic  bipinnaria  and  bra- 
chiolaria larvae  (Mortensen,  1921;  Crump,  1971).  These 
feeding  larvae  are  typical  of  the  Asteroidea  and  are  con- 
sidered to  be  of  great  antiquity  (Strathmann,  1978a).  In 
contrast,  all  the  Australian  species  examined  thus  far  are 
direct  developers.  P.  calcar,  P.  pseudoexigua,  and  P.  gunnii 
have  large  yolky  eggs  and  develop  directly  through  a  non- 
feeding  planktonic  brachiolaria  (Lawson-Kerr  and  An- 
derson, 1978;  Grice  and  Lethbridge,  1989;  Byrne,  1991; 
Chen  and  Chen,  1991).  P.  exigua  oviposits  large  eggs  that 
develop  through  a  modified  benthic  brachiolaria  (Lawson- 
Kerr  and  Anderson,  1978;  Byrne,  1991).  At  the  end  of 
the  indirect-direct  continuum  of  development  exhibited 


332 


ASTEROID  DEVELOPMENT 


333 


Table  I 

Life  history  /raits  of  Patiriella  species  from  Australia 
and  New  Zealand* 

Oocyte 
diameter 
Species  (mm)        Developmental  pattern 


Larvae 


P.  regitlaris  150         Indirect/planktotrophic     Bipinnaria  and 

brachiolaria 
P.  gunnii  360         Direct/lecithotrophic         Planktonic 

brachiolaria 
P.  calcar  400         Direct/lecitholrophic         Planktonic 

brachiolaria 
P.  pseudoexigua  Direct/lecithotrophic        Planktonic 

brachiolaria 
P.  exigua  400         Direct/lecithotrophic         Benthic 

brachiolaria 
P.  vivipara  120         Direct/viviparous  Intraovarian 

brooder 
No  larva 
P.  parvivipara  100         Direct/viviparous  Intraovanan 

brooder 
No  larva 

*  Data  from:  Dartnall  (1969);  Crump  (1971);  Keough  and  Dartnall 
(1978);  Lawson-Kerr  and  Anderson  (1978);  Byrne  (1991);  Chen  and 
Chen  (1991). 


by  Patiriella.  are  the  intraovarian  brooders,  P.  vivipara 
and  P.  parvivipara,  which  give  birth  to  crawl-away  juve- 
niles (Dartnall,  1969;  Chia,  1976;  Keough  and  Dartnall, 
1978;  Byrne,  1991). 

Several  nomenclatural  systems  have  been  suggested  for 
the  diverse  developmental  patterns  in  the  Asteroidea 
(Chia,  1968.  1974;  Oguro  et  al..  1976,  1988).  In  one  sys- 
tem, development  through  a  bipinnaria  and  brachiolaria 
larvae  is  termed  indirect,  whereas  development  only 
through  a  brachiolaria  is  termed  direct  (for  review,  Oguro 
et  al.,  1988).  This  system  is  most  appropriate  for  Patiriella. 
Other  systems  make  the  distinction  between  indirect- 
planktotrophic  larvae  with  a  functional  gut  and  direct- 
lecithotrophic  larvae  without  a  functional  gut  (Chia, 
1968).  The  recent  finding,  however,  of  an  intermediate 
pattern  of  asteroid  development,  through  a  larva  that  has 
both  planktotrophic  and  lecithotrophic  features,  obscures 
this  distinction  (Bosch,  1989). 

Comparative  embryology  of  closely  related  species  is  a 
powerful  tool  for  the  investigation  of  developmental  pro- 
cesses in  evolution  because  homologous  characters  can 
be  compared  (Raff,  1987).  This  approach  has  attracted 
renewed  interest,  particularly  with  respect  to  echinoids, 
where  recent  studies  have  revealed  that  direct  development 
arose  through  heterochronies  in  the  appearance  of  adult 
features  (Raff,  1987;  Wray  and  Raff,  1989).  Heterochro- 
nies, changes  in  the  relative  timing  or  rate  of  ontogenic 
events,  are  considered  to  be  an  important  means  of  ef- 


fecting evolutionary  change  (Anderson,  1987).  The  range 
of  life  histories  in  Patiriella  listed  in  Table  I  presents  an 
ideal  system  with  which  to  investigate  the  modifications 
involved  with  the  shift  to  direct  development  within  a 
monogeneric  group.  In  P.  vivipara,  direct  development  is 
achieved  by  heterochrony  in  suppression  of  larval  char- 
acters and  accelerated  development  of  adult  features 
(Byrne,  1991). 

In  the  evolutionary  sequence  of  developmental  change 
in  Patiriella,  the  planktotrophic  development  of  P.  reg- 
ularis  represents  the  ancestral  mode  of  development  in 
the  genus,  and,  in  this  investigation,  is  described  in  detail. 
Particular  attention  is  paid  to  the  pattern  of  larval  ciliation 
and  the  structure  of  the  larval  arms,  features  often  mod- 
ified in  lecithotrophic  larvae  (Strathmann,  1978a).  Settle- 
ment behavior  and  metamorphosis  are  also  described.  The 
ontogeny  of  P.  regularis  will  provide  the  chronological 


Table  II 

Chronology  of  development  of  Patiriella  regularis  at  I8-22°C 


Time 


Stage 


0  Fertilization 

15-60  s  Elevation  of  fertilization  membrane 

40-60  mm       First  cleavage 

1-1.5  h  Second  cleavage 

2-2.5  h  Third  cleavage 

3  h  Fourth  cleavage 

3.5-5  h  Early  blastula 

6-9  h  Wrinkled  blastula 

15-17.5  h         Late  blastula/early  gastrula 

25  h  Hatching,  gastrula  with  elongating  archenteron 

30-35  h  Advanced  gastrula,  budding  of  mesenchyme  cells  from 

terminal  expansion  of  archenteron 

45-55  h  Early  bipinnaria,  enterocoel  and  stomodeum  formation, 

archenteron  bent  towards  oral  surface  to  complete 
gut,  the  posterior  enterocoel  starts  as  a  thickening  of 
the  left  side  of  the  archenteron 

55-60  h  Bipinnaria.  ciliary  bands  distinct,  gut  regions 

differentiate 

65-75  h  Bipinnarial  arms  well-developed,  posterior  enterocoel  is 

vesicular,  posterior  elongation  of  right  and  left 
enterocoels,  hydropore  open,  fusion  of  left  enterocoel 
with  posterior  enterocoel 

4-5  days          Anterior  growth  of  enterocoels  into  oral  hood  and 
fusion  to  form  the  axohydrocoel 

6  days  Extension  of  axohydrocoel  into  oral  hood,  right  and  left 

enterocoels  continue  to  grow  posteriorly,  posterior 
enterocoel  forms  part  of  the  left  posterior  coelom, 
formation  of  the  ventral  horn 

10-14  days      Advanced  bipinnaria,  fusion  of  the  ventral  horn  with 
the  right  enterocoel,  the  axohydrocoel  with  two 
lateral  extensions  for  brachia 

4-6  weeks        Early  brachiolaria,  growing  brachiolar  arms 

8  weeks  Advanced  brachiolaria,  arms  and  adhesive  disc  well- 

developed,  five  lobes  of  the  hydrocoel  present, 
formation  of  adult  primordium  and  skeleton 

9-10  weeks      Larvae  competent  to  metamorphose 


334 


M    BYRNE  AND  M.  F.  BARKER 


m 


n 


Figure  1 .  Development  through  the  bipinnaria  stage,  a.  Egg  shortly  after  fertilization  with  an  elevated 
fertilization  membrane  and  one  polar  body  (arrowhead),  b.  One  hour,  first  cleavage,  two  polar  bodies  are 
evident  (arrowhead),  c.  One  and  a  half  hours,  second  cleavage,  d.  Four  hours,  early  blastula.  e.  Eight 
hours.wrinkled  blastula.  f.  Sixteen  hours,  late  coeloblastula  rotating  in  membrane,  g.  Twenty-five  hours. 


ASTEROID  DEVELOPMENT 


335 


basis  required  to  determine  the  morphological  and  het- 
erochronic  changes  underlying  the  evolution  of  direct  de- 
velopment within  the  genus. 

Materials  and  Methods 

Specimens  of  Patiriella  regularis  were  collected  during 
slack  water  at  5-10  m  depth  from  Otago  Harbour,  New 
Zealand  (45 °49.7'S;  170° 38.4'E),  near  the  Portobello  Ma- 
rine Laboratory,  in  January  1990.  This  species  is  gono- 
choric,  and  ovaries  and  testes  were  dissected  from  mature 
specimens.  The  testes  were  stored  dry  at  4°C,  and  the 
ovaries  were  placed  in  a  10~5  M  solution  of  1-methylad- 
enine  in  filtered  seawater  (Kanatani,  1969).  Following 
ovulation,  the  ova  were  transferred  to  beakers  of  1 .0  ^m 
filtered  seawater  and  washed  gently  through  several 
changes  of  filtered  seawater.  For  fertilization,  the  eggs  were 
placed  approximately  one  cell  deep  in  250-ml  beakers, 
and  a  few  drops  of  diluted  sperm  were  added.  After  5-10 
min,  the  eggs  were  washed  in  fresh  seawater  to  remove 
excess  sperm,  and  the  fertilized  eggs  were  placed  in  5-1 
beakers  of  filtered  seawater.  The  cultures  were  stirred 
gently  by  motor-driven  paddles.  When  the  embryos  at- 
tained the  swimming  gastrula  stage,  the  cultures  were  fil- 
tered and  placed  in  fresh  seawater.  Thereafter,  the  seawater 
in  the  cultures  was  changed  once  a  week.  Just  prior  to  the 
early  bipinnaria  stage,  feeding  of  the  larvae  commenced. 
The  larvae  were  fed  from  unialgal  cultures  of  the  flagellate 
Dunaliella  primolecta  Butcher.  The  temperature  of  the 
culture  room  ranged  from  18°  to  22°C. 

Embryogenesis  and  larval  development  of  Patiriella 
regularis  were  documented  by  light  and  scanning  electron 
microscopy  (SEM).  For  SEM,  the  embryos  and  larvae 
were  fixed  in  2.5%  glutaraldehyde  in  0.45  ^m  filtered  sea- 
water  for  1  h  at  room  temperature.  Once  the  bipinnaria 
stage  was  attained,  larvae  fixed  by  this  method  were  first 
relaxed  in  6.8%  MgCl2  in  distilled  water  before  being 
placed  in  the  primary  fixative.  Following  primary  fixation, 
the  specimens  were  washed  in  2.5%  sodium  bicarbonate 
(pH  7.2)  and  post-fixed  in  2%  OsO4  in  1.25%  sodium 
bicarbonate  for  1  h  at  room  temperature.  The  specimens 
were  then  washed  in  distilled  water  and  dehydrated  in 


ethanol.  After  dehydration,  the  specimens  were  critical 
point  dried,  sputter  coated,  and  viewed  with  a  Joel  JSM- 
35C  scanning  electron  microscope.  In  addition,  the  fix- 
ation method  of  Barker  ( 1978a)  was  also  used.  According 
to  this  method,  larvae  placed  in  a  small  drop  of  seawater 
were  initially  fixed  by  the  addition  of  Bouin's  fluid.  The 
larvae  were  then  transferred  to  3%  glutaraldehyde  in  0.2 
Mcacodylate  buffer  for  1  h  at  room  temperature.  Follow- 
ing a  rinse  in  the  same  buffer,  the  specimens  were  post- 
fixed  in  2%  OsO4  in  cacodylate  buffer.  Although  the  in- 
troduction of  Bouin's  caused  the  larvae  to  contract  slightly, 
this  method  resulted  in  good  preservation  of  the  extra- 
cellular coat  of  the  larvae.  After  fixation,  the  larvae  were 
rinsed  in  distilled  water  and  processed  as  described  above. 


Results 


Spawning 


Spawning  of  Patiriella  regularis  was  observed  in  situ 
on  25  January  1990.  Approximately  20  individuals,  both 
males  and  females,  were  observed  releasing  gametes.  The 
sperm  exited  from  the  gonopores  as  a  narrow  plume  that 
dissipated  5-10  cm  above  the  spawning  individual.  For 
the  females,  the  eggs  rolled  on  to  the  aboral  surface  after 
exiting  from  the  gonopore.  The  shortest  distance  between 
spawning  individuals  ranged  from  0.5  to  1 .0  m,  while  the 
longest  distance  ranged  from  4  to  5  m.  These  observations 
were  recorded  during  the  day  under  sunlit  conditions  and 
coincided  with  slack  water. 

In  the  laboratory,  the  ovaries  of  Patiriella  regularis  ex- 
hibited a  long  hormone-dependent  period.  It  took  3-5  h 
before  oocyte  maturation;  ovulation  and  spawning  was 
induced  by  1-methyladenine.  The  spawned  ova  were  green 
and  150  /tm  in  diameter  (±9  jim;  n  =  20). 

Embryogenesis 

The  chronology  of  the  development  of  Patiriella  reg- 
ularis is  outlined  in  Table  II.  A  fertilization  membrane 
forms  1 5-60  s  after  the  introduction  of  sperm  into  the 
beakers  containing  ova,  and  the  two  polar  bodies  are  given 
off  within  20  min  (Fig.  la,  b).  Cleavage  is  radial  and  ho- 


hatching  gastrulae.  B,  blastopore:  F.  fertilization  membrane,  h.  Twenty-five  hours,  swimming  gastrula.  i. 
Thirty-five  hours,  advanced  gastrula,  mesenchyme  cells  (M)  are  budding  off  into  the  blastocoel.  A,  archenteron.  j. 
Fourty-five  hours,  early  bipinnaria,  right  and  left  enterocoels  are  starting  to  form  (C).  A,  archenteron.  k. 
Fourty-seven  hours,  bipinnaria,  enterocoels  are  forming  (C),  archenteron  is  complete.  A  small  bulge  on  the 
archenteron  is  the  beginning  of  the  posterior  enterocoel  (P).  I.  Fifty-five  hours,  the  right  (RC)  and  left 
enterocoels  grow  posteriorly.  On  the  left  side  of  the  achenteron,  a  small  group  of  cells  form  the  posterior 
enterocoel  (P).  m.  Seventy  hours,  dorsal  view.  LC,  left  enterocoels;  M,  mouth;  P,  posterior  enterocoel;  O, 
oesophagus;  S,  stomach,  n.  Seventy  hours,  bipinnaria  in  side  view,  gut  regions  and  hydropore  (H)  are  evident. 
LC,  left  anterior  enterocoel;  O,  oesophagus;  P,  posterior  enterocoel;  S,  stomach,  o.  Seventy-five  hours,  ventral 
view.  The  arrow  points  to  the  fusing  left  anterior  (LC)  and  posterior  (P)  enterocoels.  The  right  and  left 
anterior  enterocoels  have  started  to  grow  into  the  oral  hood.  Scale  bars  =  100  Mm. 


336 


M    BYRNE  AND  M.  F    BARKER 


;      o-.^X 

SiSfcSi/  • 


Figure  2.  Development  through  metamorphosis,  a.  Eighty-eight  hours,  dorsal  view.  Growth  of  the  right 
enterocoel  (RC)  into  the  oral  hood.  At  the  position  where  the  two  left  enterocoels  have  met,  tissue  derived 
from  fusion  of  their  end  walls  is  evident  (arrow),  h.  Four  and  a  half  days,  side  view  of  a  bipinnaria  showing 


ASTEROID  DEVELOPMENT 


337 


loblastic  (Fig.  Ib-d).  The  first  cell  division  occurs  40-60 
min  after  fertilization,  and  the  early  blastula  stage  is 
reached  witnin  4  h.  By  4  h,  asynchrony  was  evident  in  all 
cultures,  with  some  embryos  at  a  more  advanced  stage 
than  others.  Five  hours  post-fertilization,  the  embryos  are 
well-developed  blastulae  164  ^m  in  diameter  (±7  ^m;  n 
=  10)  (Fig.  Id).  The  blastulae  rotate  within  their  close- 
fitting  fertilization  membranes,  propelled  by  their  ciliary 
covering.  Blastular  wrinkling  starts  6  h  after  fertilization 
with  folding  of  the  blastoderm  into  the  blastocoel.  The 
furrows  of  the  wrinkled  blastulae  are  most  apparent  in 
8-h  larvae  (Fig.  le).  Subsequently,  the  furrows  smooth 
out  and  1 8-h  cultures  contain  late  blastulae  with  a  smooth 
surface  and  early  gastrulae  (Fig.  If,  g).  Hatching  ensues 
through  rupture  of  the  fertilization  membrane,  and  the 
gastrulae  become  free-swimming  larvae  (Fig.  Ig,  h).  At 
hatching,  the  larvae  are  round  to  elongate  and  have  a 
shallow  blastopore.  They  continue  to  elongate  with  growth 
of  the  archenteron  into  the  blastocoel.  The  blind  end  of 
the  archenteron  expands  and  mesenchyme  cells  detach 
from  its  tip,  moving  into  the  blastocoel  (Fig.  li).  At  this 
stage,  the  larvae  are  197  ^m  long  (±1.2  ^m,  n  =  10). 

Early  bipinnaria  are  present  by  the  end  of  the  second 
day.  The  right  and  left  enterocoels  form  as  pouches  off 
the  expanded  tip  of  the  archenteron  (Fig.  Ij).  A  shallow 
stomodeum  is  present,  and  the  blind  end  of  the  archen- 
teron bends  towards  the  oral  surface.  During  this  stage, 
the  posterior  region  bends  ventrally,  and  from  this  time 
the  blastopore  can  be  regarded  as  the  larval  anus.  By  55 
h,  the  archenteron  fuses  with  the  stomodeal  invagination, 
thereby  completing  the  larval  gut  (Fig.  Ik).  With  devel- 
opment of  the  ciliated  bands,  algal  food  was  introduced 
into  the  cultures.  The  larvae  now  have  a  distinct  peroral 
hood  region.  In  addition,  a  shallow  evagination,  destined 
to  form  the  posterior  enterocoel,  is  evident  on  the  left- 
hand  wall  of  the  archenteron;  this  soon  grows  to  form  a 
small  thickening  of  cells  (Fig.  Ik,  1). 

By  the  end  of  the  third  day,  the  bipinnaria  are  feeding 
and  have  well-defined  pre-  and  postoral  ciliary  bands.  At 
this  stage,  the  bipinnarial  processes — lateral  and  anterior 


projections  of  the  larval  body  wall — start  to  form.  The 
regions  of  the  gut  differentiate  with  the  expansion  of  the 
stomach  and  the  separation  of  the  stomach  from  the  oe- 
sophagus by  the  cardiac  sphincter  (Fig.  1m).  In  three-day- 
old  bipinnaria,  the  right  and  left  enterocoels  increase  in 
length  as  they  grow  posteriorly,  and  the  hydropore  exits 
on  the  dorsal  surface  (Fig.  In).  During  the  fourth  day  of 
development,  the  small  thickening  on  the  archenteron 
wall  grows  to  form  a  solid  ball  of  cells  attached  to  the 
stomach.  A  central  cavity  forms  in  this  structure,  thereby 
forming  a  posterior  enterocoel  on  the  left  side  of  the  larvae 
(Fig.  In,  o).  This  posterior  enterocoel  increases  in  size 
and  is  a  conspicuous  feature  of  all  the  larvae  examined 
from  five  different  cultures.  When  the  advancing  left  an- 
terior enterocoel  reaches  the  posterior  enterocoel,  the  two 
enterocoels  fuse  (Figs,  lo,  2a).  In  some  larvae,  fusion  of 
the  two  left  coelomic  pouches  was  complete  75  h  after 
fertilization.  With  subsequent  development,  it  was  evident 
that  the  posterior  enterocoel  forms  part,  if  not  all,  of  the 
left  posterior  coelom.  Where  the  two  enterocoels  meet,  a 
partition  derived  from  fusion  of  their  tissues  forms  (Fig. 
2e,  g).  During  the  fourth  day  of  development,  the  right 
and  left  enterocoels  extend  anteriorly  into  the  oral  hood 
(Figs,  lo,  2a).  The  larval  length  is  now  630  ^m  (±5.8  ^/m; 
n  =  10). 

In  4.5  day  larvae,  the  anterior  extensions  of  the  right 
and  left  enterocoels  fuse  to  form  the  axohydrocoel  in  the 
oral  hood  (Fig.  2b).  This  anterior  coelom  grows  to  form 
an  extension  into  the  hood  where  the  median-dorsal  pro- 
cess develops  (Fig.  2c-e).  Five-day-old  larvae  are  well- 
developed  bipinnaria  and  the  ciliary  tracts  increase  in 
length  following  the  edges  of  the  bipinnarial  processes. 
The  bipinnaria  exhibit  muscular  movements  including 
contraction  of  the  cardiac  sphincter  and  dorsal  and  ventral 
flexure  of  the  oral  hood,  which  results  in  broadening  and 
closure  of  the  oral  cavity  (Fig.  2c).  Internally,  the  fused 
left  enterocoels  extend  below  the  gut,  while  growth  of  the 
right  enterocoel  is  slower.  The  partition  derived  from  fu- 
sion of  the  two  left  enterocoels  divides  the  left  enterocoel 
into  anterior  and  posterior  regions  (Fig.  2e,  g).  Partition 


the  anterior  coelom  (arrow)  in  the  oral  hood  formed  through  fusion  of  the  right  and  left  enterocoels.  H. 
hydropore.  c.  Six  days,  bipmnaria  from  the  side,  exhibiting  dorsal  flexure,  the  anterior  coelom  has  grown 
into  the  oral  hood  (arrow).  I.  intestine.  O,  oesophagus,  S,  stomach,  d.  Four-week-old  culture  containing  late 
bipinnaria.  e.  Ten  days,  dorsal  view,  late  bipinnaria/early  brachiolaria.  The  anterior  coelom  (AC)  has  grown 
to  form  the  lumen  of  the  future  median  brachium,  two  small  lateral  branches  at  the  base  of  this  coelom  (L) 
are  destined  to  be  the  coelomic  lumina  of  the  posterior  brachia.  The  left  posterior  enterocoel  (P)  has  grown 
below  the  gut.  A  septum-like  structure  (arrow),  partitions  the  left  coelom  into  anterior  and  posterior  sections, 
f.  Four  weeks,  ventral  view,  early  brachiolaria,  the  ventral  horn  (V)  of  the  left  posterior  coelom  has  grown 
around  the  gut  and  fused  with  the  right  enterocoel.  M,  mouth,  g.  Four  weeks,  detail  of  the  septum  (arrow) 
dividing  the  left  anterior  (LC)  and  posterior  coelom  (P).  RC,  right  enterocoel,  S.  stomach,  h.  Eight  weeks, 
ventral  view,  late  brachiolaria,  the  median  (MB)  and  posterior  (PB)  brachia  and  the  adhesive  disc  (AD)  are 
well-developed.  The  lobes  of  the  hydrocoel  (He)  are  evident,  i.  Eight  weeks,  ventral  view,  adult  pnmordium 
of  a  late  brachiolana,  primary  spicules  (S)  lie  along  the  lobes  of  the  hydrocoel.  j.  Nine  weeks,  metamorphosing 
larva  from  the  aboral  surface.  S,  skeleton.  Scale  bars:  a,b,c,g,i,j  =  100  ^m.  Scale  bars:  d.e.f.h;  =  150  ^m. 


Figure  3.  SEM  of  development  through  the  bipinnaria  stage,  a.  Late  blastula  within  the  fertilization 
membrane,  b.  Hatching  and  newly  hatched  gastrulae.  B.  blastopore;  F.  fertilization  membrane,  c.  Gastrula 
starting  to  elongate,  d.  Elongate  gastrula.  e.  Early  bipinnaria  forming  the  stomodeal  invagination  (S).  f. 
Bipinnanal  shape  starting  to  develop,  the  stomodeum  (S)  has  enlarged  and  the  blaslopore  has  moved  to  a 
ventral  position  to  form  the  anus  (A).  O,  forming  oral  hood.  g.  Bipinnaria  side  view  with  a  distinct  oral 
hood  (O)  and  ciliary  tracts  (C).  h.  Late  bipinnaria  ventral  view  with  oral-  (OC)  and  postoral  (PC)  ciliary 
tracts.  M,  mouth,  i.  Ciliary  field  covering  gastrula.  j.  Detail  of  bipinnaria  in  Figure  3h,  showing  the  ciliary 
tracts  around  mouth  and  scattered  cilia  covering  the  larva,  k.  Ciliary  tract  (C)  of  a  bipinnaria.  1.  Bipinnaria 
fixed  in  the  dorsally-llexed  position  showing  the  plug-like  structure  on  the  post-oral  surface  (arrow).  A.  anus; 
M.  mouth;  C,  ciliary  tract.  Scales:  Fig.  3a-h.  I  =  50  ^m;  Fig.  3i-k  =  20  ^m. 

338 


ASTEROID  DEVELOPMENT 


339 


of  the  right  enterocoel  was  not  observed.  By  day  6,  the 
ventral  horn  of  the  left  posterior  coelom  forms  and  extends 
between  the  stomach  and  the  intestine.  Eight-day-old  lar- 
vae are  790  ^m  in  length  (±85  /^m;  n  =  20). 

Ten-day-old  larvae  are  advanced  bipinnaria  (length,  990 
±  1 50  /*m;  n  =  20).  By  day  1 4,  the  ventral  horn  completes 
its  growth  fusing  with  the  right  enterocoel  (Fig.  2f ).  In  the 
preoral  hood,  the  coelom  extends  anteriorly  beyond  the 
median-dorsal  process,  forming  the  lumen  of  the  future 
median  brachiolar  arm.  At  the  base  of  the  median-dorsal 
process,  the  anterior  coelom  gives  rise  to  two  lateral  ex- 
tensions destined  to  be  the  coelomic  lumina  of  the  pos- 
terior brachiolar  arms  (Fig.  2e). 

The  larvae  grow  as  advanced  bipinnaria  through  the 
first  month  of  development.  By  week  five,  early  brachio- 
laria  are  present  with  three  brachiolar  arms  or  brachia. 
The  longest  brachium  extends  from  the  median-dorsal 
process  and  contains  the  main  branch  of  the  anterior  coe- 
lom. On  either  side  are  two  small  brachia  into  which  the 
lateral  coelomic  extensions  grow.  Each  of  these  brachiolar 
arms  are  contractile.  Advanced  brachiolaria  were  present 
in  eight-week-old  cultures  (Fig.  2h).  These  larvae  have  a 
well-developed  brachiolar  complex  comprised  of  the  three 
brachia  and  a  centrally  located  adhesive  disc.  The  adult 
primordium  develops  the  posterior  region  of  the  brachio- 
laria (Fig.  2h,  i).  On  the  left  side  of  the  larvae,  the  five 
lobes  of  the  hydrocoel  are  evident  (Fig.  2h).  The  first  adult 
spicules  form  as  small  rods  positioned  along  each  lobe  of 
the  hydrocoel  (Fig.  2i).  By  week  nine,  the  larvae  were 
competent  to  metamorphose  at  a  length  of  1 430  nm  (±  1 94 
^m;  n  =  20).  This  appears  to  be  the  upper  growth  limit 
of  the  larvae,  as  three-month-old  brachiolaria  were  similar 
in  length. 

Metamorphosis 

Advanced  brachiolaria  extend  their  arms  and  attach 
them  to  the  bottom  of  the  culture  dishes  in  what  appears 
to  be  searching  behavior.  The  large  median  brachium 
bends  at  a  90°  angle  to  the  larval  body,  bringing  the  two 
posterior  brachia  and  the  adhesive  disc  into  contact  with 
the  substratum.  The  larva  then  adhere  temporarily  to  the 
bottom  of  the  dish  by  means  of  the  arm  and  then  detach 
and  continue  swimming.  To  induce  metamorphosis,  glass 
slides  with  a  primary  algal  film  or  natural  shell  substrata 
were  placed  in  finger  bowls,  and  competent  larvae  were 
introduced.  The  brachiolaria  did  not  respond  to  the  slides, 
but  attached  to  the  undersurfaces  of  the  shells  within  a 
few  hours  of  introduction.  During  temporary  attachment, 
larvae  moved  over  the  surface  of  the  substratum  and  ex- 
hibited searching  behavior,  with  the  brachiolar  arms  at- 
taching and  detaching  as  the  larvae  "walked"  over  the 
substratum.  Following  this  exploratory  phase,  the  larvae 
ceased  to  move,  attached  permanently  with  their  brachia 


and  adhesive  disc,  and  started  to  metamorphose.  During 
metamorphosis,  the  larval  body  is  shortened  and  resorbed 
to  a  thin  stalk.  The  adult  primordium  develops  with  for- 
mation of  a  pentamerous  shape.  The  hydrocoel  expands, 
and  the  first  adult  tube  feet  form  on  the  oral  surface.  These 
tube  feet  are  used  for  attachment  and  locomotion.  Even- 
tually, the  post-larvae  break  free  of  their  attachment  stalks 
taking  up  an  independent  existence  at  a  diameter  of 450- 
500  /urn  five  to  six  days  after  settlement  (Fig.  2j).  Devel- 
opment continues  with  completion  of  the  adult  digestive 
tract.  Newly  detached  post-larvae  do  not  have  a  mouth 
or  an  anus. 

Scanning  electron  microscopy 

Examination  of  the  surface  of  hatching  gastrulae  ofPa- 
tiriella  regiilaris  shows  that  they  are  covered  by  a  uniform 
field  of  cilia  (Fig.  3b).  The  wrinkled  appearance  of  the 
fertilization  membrane  is  probably  due  to  the  collapse  of 
the  membrane  during  fixation  and  drying  (Fig.  3a,  b).  On 
hatching,  the  gastrulae  start  to  elongate  (Fig.  3b-d,  i).  With 
the  development  of  the  bipinnaria,  pre-  and  postoral  por- 
tions of  the  larvae  are  evident  with  a  slight  depression 
between  them  where  the  stomodeal  invagination  arises 
(Fig.  3e,  f ).  As  the  larvae  grow,  the  bipinnarial  processes 
and  the  pre-  and  postoral  ciliary  tracts  form  (Fig.  3g,  h). 
These  tracts,  a  conspicuous  feature  of  the  larvae,  are  sin- 
uous ridges  of  dense  cilia  that  follow  the  contours  of  the 
bipinnarial  processes  (Fig.  3j,  k).  In  addition  to  the  ciliary 
tracts,  the  bipinnaria  are  also  covered  by  a  uniform  field 
of  cilia  (Fig.  3j,  k).  Bipinnaria  preserved  in  the  dorsally 
flexed  position  reveal  the  presence  of  a  plug-like  structure 
on  the  postoral  surface  (Fig.  31).  On  contraction  of  the 
larva,  this  structure  would  function  as  a  seal  over  the 
mouth. 

Formation  of  the  brachiolar  complex  is  evident  with 
the  appearance  of  the  median  brachiolar  arm  and  two 
small  lateral  projections  (Fig.  4a).  Ridges  on  the  median 
arm  are  developing  papillae  (Fig.  4a).  In  advanced  bra- 
chiolaria, the  arms  take  on  their  distinctive  shape  and  are 
covered  by  adhesive  papillae  (Fig.  4b,  c).  An  adhesive  disc 
is  positioned  at  the  base  of  the  arms  (Fig.  5a).  Like  the 
bipinnaria,  the  brachiolaria  has  ciliary  tracts  and  is  covered 
by  cilia  (Fig.  4b,  c).  In  addition  to  the  preoral  ciliary  tract 
on  the  median-dorsal  process,  a  lateral  ciliary  tract  is 
present  along  the  median  brachium  (Fig.  4b,  i,  j). 

Preservation  of  the  brachiolaria  larvae  differed  with  the 
two  fixation  methods  used.  An  external  coat  covers  the 
adhesive  surface  of  the  arms  of  brachiolaria  fixed  with  the 
Bouin's  method  (Fig.  4a-j),  whereas  this  coat  is  not  present 
in  larvae  fixed  with  the  glutaraldehyde-seawater  method 
(Fig.  5a-h).  The  coat  is  a  thin  mesh-like  material  on  the 
surface  of  the  brachia  that  gives  the  arms  a  smooth  ap- 
pearance (Fig.  4b-j).  Due  to  contraction  of  the  larvae,  it 


340 


M.  BYRNE  AND  M.  F.  BARKER 


Figure  4.  SEM  of  development  through  the  brachiolana  stage  of  larvae  fixed  initially  with  Bouin's.  a. 
Early  brachiolana.  The  two  posterior  arms  (PB)  are  starting  to  form  at  the  base  of  the  median  brachium 
(MB).  Ridges  (arrow)  on  the  surface  of  the  median  brachium  are  developing  papillae,  b.  Brachiolana  side 
view.  The  median  brachium  (MB)  emerges  from  the  median-dorsal  process  (M)  and  the  posterior  brachia 
(PB).  A,  developing  adult  primordium;  C.  ciliary  tract,  c.  Late  brachiolana  ventral  view.  Note  the  smooth 
surface  of  the  median  brachium  (MB),  which  has  two  rows  of  papillae  (P),  and  the  cilia  covering  of  the 
larva.  The  adult  primordial  (A)  region  is  evident  posteriorly.  PB.  posterior  brachium.  d.  Detail  of  the  bra- 
chiolaria  shown  in  Figure  2b.  The  posterior  brachium  has  a  smooth  extracellular  coat  through  which  cilia 
(C)  emerge,  e.  Detail  of  the  advanced  brachiolana  shown  in  Figure  2c  showing  the  posterior  brachium  and 


ASTEROID  DEVELOPMENT 


341 


is  not  possible  to  determine  whether  the  adhesive  disc  has 
an  extracellular  covering.  The  rest  of  the  brachiolar  surface 
does  not  have  this  coat  (Fig.  4b-j). 

The  removal  of  the  extracellular  coat  by  the  glutaral- 
dehyde-seawater  method  reveals  the  underlying  structure 
of  the  brachiolar  complex  (Fig.  5a-g).  Papillae  cover  the 
brachia  down  to  their  bases  and  surround  the  adhesive 
disc  (Fig.  5a,  b).  The  median  brachium  is  considerably 
longer  than  the  other  two  and  has  9-16  papillae  arranged 
in  two  rows.  A  cluster  of  papillae  covers  the  surface  of 
the  posterior  arms  (Fig.  5c).  Nodular  arrays  of  raised  ep- 
ithelial cells  dot  the  surface  of  the  papillae  (Fig.  5b,d-f). 
In  side-view,  these  nodules  are  raised  structures  that  have 
a  fuzzy  tip,  probably  comprised  of  microvilli  (Fig.  5e).  In 
brachiolaria  with  an  intact  extracellular  coat,  small  ele- 
vations of  the  coat  indicate  the  position  of  the  underlying 
nodules  (Fig.  4g,  h).  Cilia  on  and  around  the  papillae  oc- 
casionally protrude  through  the  glycocalyx  (Figs.  4d,  h: 
5d,  e).  The  adhesive  disc  is  a  round,  flat  structure  with 
raised  epithelial  cells  similar  to  those  seen  on  the  papillae 
(Fig.  5b,  g).  In  larvae  fixed  with  the  glutaraldehyde-sea- 
water  method,  smooth  patches  of  material  apparently  se- 
creted by  the  papillae  are  evident  on  the  surface  of  the 
brachia  (Fig.  5c). 

Competent  brachiolaria  have  a  distinct  adult  rudiment 
at  the  posterior  end  of  the  larvae  and  scattered  cilia  cover 
the  future  aboral  surface  (Fig.  5h).  The  reduction  of  the 
larval  body  to  a  thin  attachment  stalk  is  shown  in  the 
whispy  tissue  attached  to  the  metamorphosing  larva  in 
Figure  5i.  Post-larvae  have  two  pairs  of  tube  feet  per  arm, 
and  cilia  are  present  on  the  epidermis  (Fig.  5j). 

Discussion 

Development  of  Patiriella  regularis  is  similar  to  other 
asteroids  that  develop  indirectly  through  planktotrophic 
bipinnaria  and  brachiolaria  larvae  (Dan,  1968;  Strath- 
mann,  1987).  The  bipinnarial  processes  of  P.  regularis 
larvae,  characteristic  of  spinulosan  asteroids,  are  relatively 
short  in  comparison  with  those  of  forcipulate  larvae,  which 
develop  into  long  and  slender  extensions  of  the  larval  body 
(Gemmill,  1914;  Strathmann,  1971;  Barker,  1978b). 

The  wrinkled  blastula  has  been  widely  reported  in  as- 
teroid embryology  for  both  indirect  and  direct  developers 
(Mortensen,  1921;  Chia,  1968;  Komatsu,  1972,  1976; 


Oguro  el  a!..  1976;  Byrne,  1991).  A  wrinkled  blastula  oc- 
curs in  Patiriella  regularis.  which  has  small  eggs,  and  it 
also  occurs  in  the  Australian  species,  P.  exigua.  P.  ca/car, 
and  P.  gunnii.  which  have  large  ova  350-400  fjm  in  di- 
ameter (Lawson-Kerr  and  Anderson,  1978;  Byrne,  1991). 
Blastular  wrinkling  in  each  of  these  Patiriella  species  re- 
sults from  the  folding  of  the  blastoderm  into  the  blastocoel 
with  subsequent  smoothing  out  at  the  advanced  blastula 
stage  (Lawson-Kerr  and  Anderson,  1978;  Byrne,  1991). 
In  echinoids,  wrinkled  blastulae  are  only  reported  in  spe- 
cies with  large  eggs  (Williams  and  Anderson,  1975;  Ame- 
miya  and  Tsuchiya,  1979;  Raff,  1987;  Parks  el  al.  1989), 
and  the  wrinkled  blastula  may  be  a  consequence  of  the 
shift  from  indirect  to  direct  development  (Raff,  1987; 
Parks  el  ui.  1989).  There  is  no  evidence  of  a  relationship 
between  blastular  wrinkling  and  egg  size  in  asteroids,  and 
infolding  of  the  blastoderm  may  be  associated  with  the 
mechanics  of  cleavage  (Anderson,  pers.  comm.).  Up  to 
the  early  blastula  stage,  cleavage  gives  rise  to  large  cuboidal 
blastomeres  held  within  a  close-fitting  fertilization  mem- 
brane. As  development  continues,  the  embryo  may  not 
be  able  to  accommodate  additional  cuboidal  cells  in  a 
spherical  shape  due  to  insufficient  space  within  the  fertil- 
ization membrane,  resulting  in  the  onset  of  wrinkling.  In 
asteroids  and  echinoids  that  have  a  wrinkled  blastula  in 
their  development,  smoothing  of  advanced  blastula  cor- 
responds with  the  transition  from  a  cuboidal  to  a  colum- 
nar blastomere  organization  (Parks  el  al.,  1989;  Byrne, 
pers.  obs.).  Compared  with  cuboidal  blastomeres,  this  co- 
lumnar organization  may  be  more  readily  accommodated 
in  a  spherical  shape.  But  not  all  asteroids  have  a  wrinkled 
blastula  (Dan,  1968;  Strathmann,  1987),  and  for  these 
species,  the  spatial  relationship  between  the  blastular  sur- 
face and  the  fertilization  membrane  during  the  cuboidal- 
columnar  transition  should  be  documented.  Recent  work 
suggests  that  wrinkling  of  lecithotrophic  echinoid  embryos 
may  also  be  a  mechanical  phenomenon  (Henry,  pers. 
comm.). 

The  posterior  enterocoel  that  forms  on  the  left  side  of 
the  archenteron  in  the  early  bipinnaria  of  Patiriella  regu- 
laris is  not  a  general  feature  of  asteroid  embryology  (Dan, 
1968;  Strathmann,  1987).  Homologous  structures  are  re- 
ported in  the  bipinnaria  ofAsterias  rubens  and  Marthas- 
terias  glacialis,  where  similar  masses  of  cells  may  arise 
on  the  right  or  left  side  of  the  archenteron  (Gemmill. 


papillae  (P).The  arm  has  an  extracellular  coat  that  gives  it  a  smooth  appearance,  f.  Median  brachium  and 
papillae  (P).  The  arm  has  an  extracellular  coat  that  gives  it  a  smooth  appearance,  g.  Detail  of  the  papillae 
of  the  medium  brachium  and  the  smooth  mesh-like  extracellular  coat.  Raised  bumps  on  the  papillae  (arrow) 
indicate  the  position  of  underlying  raised  epithelial  cells  (see  Fig.  5b).  h.  Papillae  at  the  tip  of  the  median 
brachium  with  cilia  (C)  emerging  through  the  surface  coat.  i.  Anterior  portion  of  brachiolaria  shown  in 
Figure  2b,  showing  the  pre-oral  ciliary  tract  on  the  median  dorsal  process  (arrow)  and  the  ciliary  tract  (C) 
on  the  median  brachium.  j.  Detail  of  the  ciliary  tract  (C)  along  the  median  brachium.  Scales:  Fig.  4a,  d-j 
=  20  Mm;  Fig.  4b.  c  =  100  ^m. 


Figure  5.  SEM  of  development  through  metamorphosis  of  larvae  fixed  by  the  glutaraldehyde-seawater 
method,  a.  Brachiolana  ventral  view.  Note  the  median  (MB)  and  posterior  brachia  (PB)  and  the  adhesive 
disc  (AD).  Cilia  cover  the  larva,  and  the  adult  primordium  is  evident  posteriorly  (A),  b.  Median  brachium 
(MB)  with  two  rows  of  papillae.  Note  the  absence  of  an  extracellular  coat.  The  papillae  and  adhesive  disc 
(AD)  are  dotted  by  raised  epithelial  cells,  c.  Posterior  brachium  with  a  patch  of  secreted  material  on  its 
surface  (arrow).  AD,  Adhesive  disc.  d.  Detail  of  the  brachiolaria  shown  in  Figure  5a.  Cilia  cover  the  larva 
and  are  also  present  on  and  around  the  papillae  (C).  Note  the  absence  of  an  extracellular  coat  on  the  posterior 

342 


ASTEROID  DEVELOPMENT 


343 


1914).  In  these  species,  this  cell  mass  detaches  from  the 
gut  and  either  breaks  up  into  mesoderm  or  fuses  with  the 
advancing  right  or  left  enterocoel;  in  M.  glacialis  a  central 
cavity  occasionally  forms  (Gemmill,  1914).  A  similar  sit- 
uation to  that  seen  in  P.  regularis  occurs  in  the  other 
asterinid  species  Asterina  miniata  and  A.  pectinifera 
(Heath.  1917;  Newman,  1925;  Komatsu,  pers.  comm.). 
As  in  P.  regularis,  a  posterior  enterocoelic  growth  arises 
on  the  left  side  of  the  archenteron  of  A.  miniata  and  A. 
pectinifera,  forming  a  third  enterocoel  that  fuses  with  the 
anterior  left  enterocoel  (Heath,  1917;Newman,  1925).  In 
P.  regularis  and  ,-1.  miniata,  the  posterior  enterocoel  is  a 
functionally  important  structure  that  grows  during  de- 
velopment and  gives  rise  to  the  posterior  coelom  (New- 
man, 1925).  This  contrasts  with  typical  asteroid  devel- 
opment, where  the  posterior  coelom  is  derived  from  par- 
tition of  the  left  anterior  enterocoel  (Gemmill,  1914;  Dan, 
1968;  Strathmann,  1987).  There  is  speculation  as  to  the 
significance  of  the  presence  of  a  posterior  enterocoelic 
growth  and  a  third  enterocoel  (Gemmill,  1914;  Heath, 
1917;  Newman,  1925).  In  the  development  of  A.  miniata, 
Newman  ( 1925)  considered  the  posterior  enterocoel  to  be 
a  vestigial  feature.  Gemmill  (1914)  and  Heath  (1917) 
considered  the  thickening  of  the  archenteron  wall  in  some 
asteroids,  and  the  third  enterocoel  in  others,  to  be  rudi- 
ments of  a  posterior  coelom  present  in  the  larvae  of  a 
common  enteropneust-echinoderm  ancestor.  Thus,  as 
suggested  by  Gemmill  (1914),  the  posterior  enterocoel  of 
P.  regularis  may  be  homologous  to  the  trunk  coelom  in 
enteropneust  embryology. 

The  plug-like  structure  on  the  post-oral  surface  of  the 
bipinnaria  of  Patiriella  regularis  has  not  been  described 
before.  This  structure  is  evident  only  in  bipinnaria  fixed 
in  the  dorsally  flexed  posture  and  appears  to  serve  as  a 
seal  for  the  mouth  on  ventral  contraction  of  the  larvae. 
In  this  manner  it  may  function  as  a  mechanism  to  prevent 
undesirable  particles  from  entering  the  mouth.  It  was  not 
seen  in  live  specimens  examined  with  the  light  microscope. 
Although  this  mouth  seal  has  not  been  reported  in  the 
bipinnaria  of  other  asteroids,  its  presence  may  be  revealed 
by  scanning  electron  microscopy. 

The  ultrastructure  of  the  brachiolar  complex  has  been 
described  for  the  forcipulate  asteroids  Stichaster  australis 
and  Coscinasterias  calamaria  (Barker,  1978a).  In  com- 
parison with  these  species,  the  median  brachiolar  arm  of 
Patiriella  regularis  is  well-supplied  with  adhesive  papillae. 


In  S.  australis  and  C.  calamaria,  adhesive  papillae  are 
limited  to  the  tip  of  the  brachia,  and  the  stem  of  the  me- 
dian brachium  is  smooth  (Barker,  1978a).  Like  the  bra- 
chiolaria  of  these  species,  cilia  are  also  present  on  the 
papillae  of  P.  regularis  and  may  have  a  sensory  role  in 
the  location  of  suitable  substrata  for  settlement  (Barker, 
1978a).  The  raised  epithelial  nodules  on  the  brachial  pa- 
pillae and  adhesive  disc  of  P.  regularis  appear  to  corre- 
spond to  the  batteries  of  secretory  cells  revealed  by  trans- 
mission electron  microscopy  of  the  brachiolar  complex 
of  S.  australis  and  C.  calamaria  (Barker,  1978a). 

Temporary  attachment  of  P.  regularis  brachiolaria  is 
achieved  by  the  median  brachium  as  it  extends  over  sub- 
stratum, assisted  by  adhesion  of  the  posterior  arms.  The 
patches  of  smooth  material  on  the  brachia  may  be  used 
for  adhesion.  For  P.  regularis,  as  reported  for  S.  australis 
and  C.  calamaria  (Barker,  1978a),  it  appears  that  per- 
manent attachment  is  achieved  by  secretion  of  a  cement- 
like  material  by  the  adhesive  disc.  Early  descriptions  of 
brachiolaria  refer  to  the  attachment  disc  as  a  'sucker'  be- 
cause it  was  thought  to  effect  attachment  by  means  of 
suction  with  the  edge  of  the  disc  forming  a  seal  (Gemmill, 
1914;  Mortensen,  1921). 

The  difference  in  preservation  of  the  brachiolaria  by 
the  two  fixation  methods  is  striking.  Brachiolaria  fixed 
initially  with  Bouin's  fluid  have  a  glycocalyx-like  material 
covering  their  brachia,  whereas  larvae  fixed  with  the  glu- 
taraldehyde-seawater  method  do  not.  Removal  of  surface 
coats  by  conventional  fixation  methods  is  reported  for 
several  echinoderms  (Cameron  and  Holland,  1983; 
McKenzie,  1987).  An  extracellular  coat  similar  to  that  on 
the  brachiolar  complex  of  P.  regularis  is  present  on  the 
larvae  of  Asterina  miniata  (Cameron  and  Holland,  1983). 
In  A.  miniata,  however,  this  coat  covers  the  entire  surface 
of  the  larva  (Cameron  and  Holland,  1983).  The  preser- 
vation of  a  glycocalyx  on  the  brachia,  but  not  on  the  rest 
of  the  larval  surface  of  P.  regularis,  suggests  that  it  may 
function  in  association  with  the  brachiolar  complex  as  a 
protective  covering  for  the  attachment  surface. 

The  searching  behavior,  settlement,  and  metamorphosis 
of Patiriella  regularis  is  characteristic  of  asteroid  brachio- 
laria (Gemmill,  1914;  Barker,  1977).  In  contrast  to  that 
reported  for  Coscinasterias  calamaria,  the  presence  of  a 
primary  algal  film  is  not  sufficient  to  induce  metamor- 
phosis of  P.  regularis  (Barker,  1977).  The  attachment  and 
metamorphosis  of  the  brachiolaria  on  the  undersides  of 


brachium.  e.  Detail  of  the  median  brachium  showing  raised  epithelial  cells  (arrows)  on  the  papillar  surface. 
Cilia  are  present  on  and  around  the  papillae  (C).  f.  Papillae  (P)  at  the  tip  of  the  median  brachium.  g.  Adhesive 
disc  (AD),  the  arrows  point  to  raised  epithelial  cells,  h.  Late  brachiolaria  ventral  view.  The  larva  is  in  the 
exploratory/attachment  posture  with  the  median  brachium  extended  90°  to  the  larval  body  (arrow).  A,  adult 
primordium.  i.  Metamorphosing  larva  detached  from  its  stalk,  which  appears  as  wispy  material  (arrow). 
Two  pairs  of  tube  feet  (T)  are  present  in  each  radius,  j.  Detail  of  the  metamorphosing  larva.  The  future 
adult  surface  is  covered  by  cilia  (C).  Scales:  Fig.  5a,  h,  i  =  50  mm;  Fig.  5b-g,  j  =  20  mm. 


344 


M.  BYRNE  AND  M.  F    BARKER 


shells  and  not  on  the  film-covered  slides,  suggests  that 
shade  and  a  rough-textured  surface  may  be  an  important 
factor  in  selecting  a  site  for  metamorphosis  in  this  species. 
Crump  (1969)  also  reported  attachment  and  metamor- 
phosis of  P.  regularis  brachiolaria  on  the  undersides  of 
introduced  substrata. 

The  observation  of  simultaneous  spawning  of  male  and 
female  Putiriella  regularis  in  the  field  is  similar  to  that 
reported  for  several  asteroids  (Minchin,  1987;  Pearse  el 
ai.  1988).  The  nearest  distance  between  spawning  P.  reg- 
ularis, however,  is  considerably  longer  than  for  Marthas- 
terias  glacialis,  which  gathers  in  spawning  assemblages 
prior  to  gamete  release  (Minchin,  1987).  Although  some 
of  the  P.  regidaris  releasing  gametes  were  0.5  m  from  an 
adjacent  spawner,  several  individuals  appeared  to  be 
spawning  in  isolation,  as  reported  by  Pearse  ct  al.  (1988). 
At  this  distance,  and  particularly  for  those  individuals 
spawning  in  isolation,  gamete  dilution  would  reduce  the 
chance  of  fertilization.  Echinoid  zygote  production  in  the 
field  decreases  dramatically  if  females  are  more  than  20 
cm  apart  from  spawning  males  (Pennington,  1985).  There 
is  evidence,  however,  that  asteroid  sperm  and  sperm  of 
other  echinoderms  are  attracted  to  conspecific  ova,  and 
this,  to  some  extent,  may  ameliorate  the  problem  of  ga- 
mete dilution  (Miller,  1989;  Byrne,  1990).  The  collection 
site  is  subject  to  strong  currents,  and  it  seems  that  the 
chances  of  fertilization  would  be  enhanced  by  the  slack 
water  conditions  that  coincided  with  spawning,  as  noted 
for  breeding  in  holothuroids  (McEuen,  1988). 

From  laboratory  culture  at  18-22°C,  the  pelagic  period 
of  Paliriella  regularis  has  a  duration  of  9-10  weeks;  it 
may  be  longer  in  the  field,  where  ambient  sea  surface 
temperatures  of  16-18°C  in  Otago  Harbour  coincide  with 
the  planktonic  period  of  this  species.  This  duration  of 
larval  life  is  similar  to  that  of  other  temperature  plank- 
totrophic  asteroids,  although  it  is  somewhat  shorter  than 
that  of  asteroids  from  the  northern  Pacific,  where  ambient 
temperatures  range  from  7  to  1 3°C  (Strathmann,  1978b). 
Latitudinal  differences  in  larval  life  undoubtedly  reflect 
differences  in  ambient  temperature,  with  the  longest 
planktonic  period  of  22  weeks  recorded  for  the  antarctic 
asteroid  Odontaster  validus  at  sea  temperatures  of  —  2- 
- 1  °C  (Pearse  and  Bosch,  1986). 

Development  of  Patiriella  regularis  through  feeding 
bipinnaria  and  brachiolaria  larvae  is  typical  of  asteroid 
embryogenesis  and  contrasts  with  the  development  of  the 
Australian  Patiriella.  P.  e.xigua,  P.  pseudoexigua,  P.  cal- 
car,  and  P.  gimnii  have  completely  lost  the  bipinnarial 
stage  and  develop  directly  through  a  non-feeding  bra- 
chiolaria ( Mortensen,  1921;  Lawson-Kerr  and  Anderson, 
1978;  Byrne,  1991;  Chen  and  Chen,  1991).  Also  in  con- 
trast to  P.  regularis,  these  species  have  large  ova,  the  evo- 
lution of  which  is  considered  to  be  a  pre-adaptive  trait 
for  the  shift  to  direct  development  (Chia,  1968).  Larvae 


derived  from  such  eggs  would  no  longer  be  obligate 
planktotrophs.  resulting  in  the  loss  of  structures  required 
for  feeding  (Strathmann.  1978a).  The  development  of  P. 
regularis  provides  the  basic  reference  for  comparison  with 
the  direct  developers.  Features  that  are  particularly  im- 
portant for  comparison  include  the  mode  and  timing  of 
archenteron  and  coelom  formation  in  P.  regularis.  and 
the  morphology  of  larval  feeding  structures.  As  docu- 
mented for  echinoids  (Raff,  1987),  the  evolution  of  direct 
development  in  Patiriella  may  involve  heterochronic 
changes  in  these  features.  Together  with  the  developmen- 
tal chronologies  of  the  other  Patiriella  species,  the  ontog- 
eny of  P.  regularis  presented  here  will  be  used  to  determine 
the  changes  underlying  the  shift  to  direct  development 
within  the  genus  and  to  assess  the  pathways  by  which 
feeding  larvae  were  lost.  The  use  of  Patiriella  as  a  tool 
with  which  to  examine  developmental  processes  in  evo- 
lution is  the  subject  of  ongoing  research. 

Acknowledgments 

We  thank  Professor  John  Jillett,  Director  of  the  Por- 
tobello  Marine  Laboratory  (PML),  for  the  use  of  facilities. 
The  staff  of  the  PML  also  provided  technical  assistance. 
In  particular  we  thank  Mr.  Michael  Stuart.  Thanks  also 
to  Ms.  Pia  Laegdsgaard  for  technical  assistance.  Special 
thanks  to  Dr.  V.  B.  Morris  for  reading  the  manuscript. 
This  work  was  supported  by  a  grant  from  the  Australian 
Research  Council. 

Literature  Cited 

Anderson,  D.  T.  1987.  Developmental  pathways  and  evolutionary  rates. 
Pp.  143-155  in  Rales  of  Evolution,  K.  S.  W.  Campbell  and  M.  F. 
Day,  eds.  Allen  and  Unwin,  Sydney. 

Amemiya,  S.,  and  T.  Tsuchiya.  1979.  Development  of  the  echinothund 
sea  urchin  Alhenosoma  ijimai.  Mar  Biol  52:  93-96. 

Barker,  M.  F.  1977.  Observations  on  the  settlement  of  the  brachiolaria 
larvae  ofStichaster  australis  (Verrill)  and  Coscinasterias  calamaria 
(Gray)  (Echinodermata:  Asteroidea)  in  the  laboratory  and  on  the 
shore.  J.  Exp.  Mar.  Biol.  Ecol.  30:  95-108. 

Barker,  M.  F.  I978a.  Structure  of  the  organs  of  attachment  of  the 
brachiolaria  larvae  of  Stichuster  australis  (Verrill)  and  Coscinasterias 
calamaria  (Gray)  (Echinodermata:  Asteroidea).  /  Exp.  Mar.  Biol. 
Ecol.  33:  1-36. 

Barker,  M.  F.  1978b.  Descriptions  of  the  larvae  ofStichaster  australis 
(Verrill)  and  Coscinasterias  calamaria  (Gray)  (Echinodermata:  As- 
teroidea) from  New  Zealand,  obtained  from  laboratory  culture.  Biol 
Bull.  154:  32-46. 

Bosch,  1.  1989.  Contrasting  modes  of  reproduction  in  two  antarctic 
asteroids  of  the  genus  Porania,  with  a  description  of  unusual  feeding 
and  non-feeding  larval  types.  Biol.  Bull  ill:  77-82. 

Byrne,  M.  1990.  Annual  reproductive  cycles  of  the  commercial  sea 
urchin  Paracentrotus  lividits  from  an  exposed  intertidal  and  a  sheltered 
subtidal  habitat  on  the  west  coast  of  Ireland.  Mar.  Biol.  104:  275- 
289. 

Byrne,  M.  1991 .  Developmental  diversity  in  the  starfish  genus  Patiriella. 
In  Proceedings  7;h  International  Echinoderm  Conference.  N.  Suzuki. 
ed.  Balkema.  Rotterdam. 


ASTEROID  DEVELOPMENT 


345 


Cameron,  R.  A.,  and  N.  D.  Holland.  1983.  Electron  microscopy  of 
extracellular  materials  during  the  development  of  a  sea  star,  Patina 
miniala  (Echinodermata:  Asteroidea).  Cell  Tissue  Res  234:  193- 
200. 

Chen,  B.,  and  C.  Chen.  1991 .  Reproductive  cycle,  larval  development, 
juvenile  growth  and  size  distribution  of  Patiriella  pscmlocxigua 
Dartnall  (Echinodermata:  Asteroidea).  In  Proceedings  "ill  Interna- 
tional Ki'luncilerm  Conference.  N.  Suzuki,  ed.  Balkema.  Rotterdam. 

Chia.  K.  S.  1968.  The  embryology  of  a  brooding  starfish,  I.eplasterias 
he.\ticti.\  (Stimpson).  Ada.  Zool.  49:  321-364. 

Chia,  F.  S.  1974.  Classification  and  adaptive  significance  of  develop- 
mental patterns  in  marine  invertebates.  Thalassia  Jugoslav.  10:  121- 
130. 

Chia,  F.  S.  1976.  Reproductive  biology  of  an  intraovarian  brooding 
starfish  Patiriella  vivipera  Dartnall.  1969.  Am.  Zoo/.  16:  181. 

Crump,  R.  G.  1969.  Aspects  of  the  biology  of  some  New  Zealand  echi- 
noderms.  Ph.D.  thesis.  University  of  Otago,  Dunedin,  New  Zealand. 

Crump,  R.  G.  1971.  Annual  reproductive  cycles  in  three  geographically 
separated  populations  of  Patiriella  regtilaris  ( Verrill),  a  common  New 
Zealand  asteroid.  /  Exp.  Mar.  Biol.  Ecol.  7:  137-162. 

Dan,  K.  1968.  Asteroidea.  Pp.  303-308  in  Invertebrate  Embryology. 
H  H.  Kume  and  K..  Dan,  eds.  NOLIT  Publishing  House.  Belgrade. 
Yugoslavia. 

Dartnall.  A.  J.  1969.  A  viviparous  species  of  Paliriella  (Asteroidea, 
Astenmdae)  from  Tasmania.  Pmc.  Linn.  Soc.  NSW  93:  294-296. 

Dartnall.  A.  J.  1971.  Australian  sea  stars  of  the  genus  Patiriella  (As- 
teroidea. Asterimdae).  Proc.  Linn.  Soc.  NSW  96:  39-51. 

Gemmill,  J.  F.  1914.  The  development  and  certain  points  in  the  adult 
structure  of  the  starfish  Asterias  ruhens.  L.  Plnlos.  Trail*  R  Soc. 
Loud  Ser  B  205:  2 1 3-294. 

Grice,  A.  J.,  and  R.  C.  Lethbridge.  1989.  Reproductive  studies  on  Pa- 
tiriella gimnii  (Asteroidea:  Asterinidae)  in  south-western  Australia. 
Auxt.  J.  Mar  Freshwater  Res,  39:  399-407. 

Heath,  H.  1917.  The  early  development  of  a  starfish  Pateria  lA.sterina) 
Mmeala.  J.  Morphol.  29:  461-469. 

kanatani,  II.  1969.  Induction  of  spawning  and  oocyte  maturation  by 
1-methyladenine  in  starfishes.  Exp.  Cell  Res.  57:  333-337. 

Keough,  M.  J.,  and  A.  J.  Dartnall.  1978.  A  new  species  of  viviparous 
asterinid  asteroid  from  Eyre  Peninsula,  South  Australia.  Ree  S.  Aust. 
Mils  17:  407-416. 

Komatsu,  M.  1972.  On  the  wrinkled  blastula  of  the  sea-star  A.ster/iiti 
pectnnfera  Zoo/.  Mag.  81:  227-231. 

Komatsu,  M.  1976.  Wrinkled  blastula  of  the  sea-star  A.sterina  minor 
Hayashi.  Dev.  Growth  Differ.  18:  435-438. 

Lawson-Kerr,  C.,  and  D.  T.  Anderson.  1978.  Reproduction,  spawning 
and  development  of  the  starfish  Patiriella  exigua  (Lamarck)  (Aster- 
oidea. Asterinidae)  and  some  comparisons  with  P.  ca/car( Lamarck). 
Aust.  J.  Mar.  Freshwater  Res.  29:  45-53. 

McEuen,  F.  S.  1988.  Spawning  behaviors  of  northeast  Pacific  sea  cu- 
cumbers (Holothuroidea:  Echinodermata).  Mar.  Biol  98:  565-585. 

McKenzie,  J.  D.  1987.  The  ultrastructure  of  the  tentacles  of  eleven 
species  of  dendrochirote  holothurians  studied  with  special  reference 
to  the  surface  coats  and  papillae.  Cell  Tissue  Res.  248:  187-199. 


Miller,  R.  L.  1989.  Evidence  for  the  presence  of  sexual  pheromones 
in  free-spawning  starfish.  ./  .Exp  Mar  Biol  Ecol.  130:205-221. 

Minchin,  D.  1987.  Sea-water  temperature  and  spawning  behaviour  in 
the  seastar  Marthastenas  glaeialis.  .Mar.  Biol.  95:  139-143. 

Mortensen,  T.  1921.  Studies  on  the  Development  and  Larval  Forms 
oj  Echinoderms  G.E.C.  Gad..  Copenhagen.  216  pp. 

Newman,  H.  H.  1925.  An  experimental  analysis  of  asymmetry  in  the 
starfish.  Paliria  mmiata  Biol.  Hull  49:  1 1 1-138. 

Oguro,  C.,  M.  Komatsu,  and  V.  T.  Kano.  1976.  Development  and 
metamorphosis  of  the  sea-star  Asteropeden  scoparius  Valenciennes. 
Biol.  Bull.  151:  560-573. 

Oguro,  C..  M.  Komatsu,  and  Y.  T.  Kano.  1988.  Significance  of  the 
nonbrachiolanan  type  of  development  in  sea-stars.  Pp.  241-246  in 
Echinoderm  Biology.  R.  Burke,  P.  Lambert,  and  R.  Parsley,  eds. 
Balkema,  Rotterdam. 

Parks,  A.  L.,  B.  W.  Bisgrove,  G.  A.  Wray,  and  R.  R.  Raff.  1989.  Direct 
development  in  the  sea  urchin  Phyllacanthiis  panispimis  (Cidaroidea): 
phylogenetic  history  and  functional  modification.  Biol.  Bull.  177: 
96-109. 

Pearse,  J.  S.,  and  I.  Bosch.  1986.  Are  the  feeding  larvae  of  the  com- 
monest antarctic  asteroid  Odonlaster  validus  really  demersal?  Bull. 
Mar  Sci  39:  477-484. 

Pearse,  J.  S.,  D.  J.  McClary,  M.  A.  Sewell,  \V.  C.  Austin,  A.  Perez- 
Ruzafa,  and  M.  Byrne,  1988.  Simultaneous  spawning  of  six  species 
of  echinoderms  in  Barkley  Sound,  British  Columbia.  Im.  Reprod. 
Dev  14:  279-288. 

Pennington,  J.  T.  1985.  The  ecology  of  fertilization  of  echinoid  eggs: 
the  consequences  of  sperm  dilution,  adult  aggregation,  and  synchro- 
nous spawning.  Biol  Bull  169:417-430. 

Raff,  R.  A.  1987.  Constraint,  flexibility,  and  phylogenetic  history  in 
the  evolution  of  direct  development  in  sea  urchins.  Dev.  Biol  119: 
6-19. 

Strathmann,  M.  F.  1987.  Reproduction  and  Development  ot  Marine 
Invertebrates  of  the  Northern  Pacific  Coast.  University  of  Washington 
Press,  Seattle.  Pp.  535-555. 

Strathmann,  R.  R.  1971.  The  feeding  behaviour  of  planktotrophic 
echinoderm  larvae:  mechanisms,  regulation,  and  rates  of  suspension- 
feeding.  J.  Exp.  Mar.  Biol  Ecol.  6:  109-160. 

Strathmann,  R.  R.  1978a.  The  evolution  and  loss  of  feeding  larval  stages 
of  marine  invertebrates.  Evolution  32:  894-906. 

Strathmann,  R.  R.  1978b.  Length  of  pelagic  period  of  echinoderms 
with  feeding  larvae  from  the  northeast  Pacific.  J.  Exp.  Mar.  Biol. 
Ecol.  34:  23-27. 

Williams,  D.  H.  C.,  and  D.  T.  Anderson.  1975.  The  reproductive  system, 
embryonic  development,  larval  development  and  metamorphosis  of 
the  sea  urchin  Heliocidans  erythrogramma  (Val.)  (Echinodermata: 
Echinometridae).  Aust.  J.  Zool.  23:  371-403. 

Wray,  G.  A.,  and  R.  A.  Raff.  1989.  Evolutionary  modification  of  cell 
lineage  in  the  direct-developing  sea  urchin  Heliocidaris  erythro- 
gramma. Dev.  Biol.  132:  458-470. 


Reference:  Uinl  Bull  180:  346-354.  (June,  1991) 


Abnormal  Sea  Urchin  Fertilization  Envelope  Assembly 

in  Low  Sodium  Seawater 

SOU-DE  CHENG1,  PATRICIA  S.  GLAS2,  AND  JEFFREY  D.  GREEN* 

Department  of  Anatomy,  Louisiana  State  University  Medical  Center,  New  Orleans,  Louisiana  70112 


Abstract.  The  structuralization  of  the  sea  urchin  fertil- 
ization envelope  (FE),  a  model  for  extracellular  macro- 
molecular  assembly,  was  found  to  require  sodium  ions, 
the  predominant  cation  of  seawater.  Eggs  from  Strongy- 
locentrotus  purpuratits  activated  in  sea  waters  with  sodium 
chloride  substitutes  (choline  or  Tris  chloride)  elevated 
incomplete  FEs.  In  addition,  the  conversion  of  the  mi- 
crovillar  casts  of  the  FE  from  blunt  (I-form)  to  angular 
(T-form)  did  not  occur.  The  permeability  of  the  abnormal 
FEs  was  also  compromised,  as  approximately  eight  times 
more  protein  than  normal  was  released  into  the  ambient 
seawater.  There  were  also  significant  increases  in  the  es- 
cape of  two  cortical  granule  (CG)  enzymes,  0-1,3-gluca- 
nase  and  ovoperoxidase.  Furthermore.  FEs  elevated  in 
choline  chloride  (ChCl)  seawater  appeared  to  be  deficient 
in  the  incorporation  of  ovoperoxidase,  an  enzyme  that  is 
normally  bound  to  the  FE  and  that  cross-links  structural 
proteins  in  the  nascent  FE.  The  morphology  of  FEs  ele- 
vated in  potassium  chloride-substituted  seawater  was 
similar  to  those  in  normal  sodium  seawater.  Thus,  it  ap- 
pears that  sodium,  or  at  least  a  similar  ion,  is  necessary 
for  the  proper  functioning  of  ovoperoxidase  and  structural 
proteins  in  the  elevation  and  normal  assembly  of  the  sea 
urchin  FE. 

Introduction 

Sea  urchin  fertilization  has  been  intensively  studied  for 
the  dramatic  intra-  and  extracellular  events  concomitant 


Received  29  August  1990:  accepted  24  January  1991. 

'  Genetics  Division.  Children's  Hospital,  300  Longwood  Avenue. 
Boston,  MA  02 11 5. 

2  Department  of  Zoology  and  Physiology.  Louisiana  State  University. 
Baton  Rouge.  LA  70803. 

Abbreviations:  SW  (artificial  seawater);  CG  (cortical  granule);  ChCl 
(choline  chloride);  FE  (fertilization  envelope);  FP  (fertilization  product); 
VL  (vitelline  layer). 

*  To  whom  correspondence  should  be  sent. 


with  the  change  from  egg  to  embryo.  The  irreversible 
transformation  of  the  relatively  thin,  soft  vitelline  layer 
(VL  =  glycocalyx),  investing  the  unfertilized  egg,  into  a 
hardened,  insoluble  fertilization  envelope  (FE),  elevated 
from  the  egg  surface,  is  a  critical  step  for  the  protection 
of  the  developing  embryo.  Furthermore,  this  process  has 
been  the  subject  of  numerous  investigations  as  an  example 
of  regulated  extracellular  matrix  assembly.  Several  recent 
reviews  (Kay  and  Shapiro.  1985;  Shapiro  et  al.,  1989; 
Somers  and  Shapiro,  1989)  contain  the  details  of  such 
investigations:  therefore,  the  process  will  be  summarized 
only  briefly  here. 

Transglutaminase  has  an  important  role  in  the  earliest 
stages  of  VL  modification  (Battaglia  and  Shapiro,  1988). 
It  is  located  on  the  egg  surface  and  catalyzes  the  incor- 
poration of  primary  amines  into  the  nascent  FE  during 
the  first  4  min  of  egg  activation.  This  process  is  apparently 
related  to  the  I-T  transition  of  the  microvillar  projections 
of  the  VL  (in  5.  purpuratiis).  because  the  transition  does 
not  occur  in  the  presence  of  transglutaminase  inhibitors. 
During  the  next  few  minutes,  ovoperoxidase  secreted  from 
the  cortical  granules  (CG)  catalyzes  the  insertion  of  struc- 
tural proteins  from  the  CGs  into  the  VL  by  cross-linking 
tyrosyl  residues  between  polypeptides  (Foerder  and  Sha- 
piro, 1977;  Hall,  1978).  Besides  this  catalytic  reaction, 
ovoperoxidase  itself  is  incorporated  into  the  nascent  FE 
via  a  specific  interaction  with  another  CG  protein,  pro- 
teoliaisin  (Weidman  et  al.,  1985).  These  enzyme  activities 
result  in  a  hardened,  insoluble,  fully  formed  FE  within 
the  first  10  min  following  egg  activation. 

Several  recent  reports  have  been  concerned  with  ionic 
requirements  for  FE  formation.  Carroll  and  Endress 
(1982)  demonstrated  that  formation  of  mature  FEs  re- 
quires the  normal  9  mM  [Ca2+]  and  48  mM  [Mg2+]  in 
the  seawater.  By  omitting  these  ions  they  could  produce 
an  "intermediate  envelope"  that  was  much  thinner  than 
the  normal  FE  and  did  not  incorporate  structural  proteins. 


346 


FERTILIZATION  ENVELOPE  ASSEMBLY 


347 


Deficiency  of  Cl  ,  the  most  abundant  anion  in  seawater, 
not  only  interferes  with  the  normal  I-T  transformation, 
but  also  increases  the  permeability  of  the  FE  (Lynn  ct  ai, 
1988:  Green  et  ai.  1990).  Furthermore,  the  normal  ex- 
ternal Na+  concentration  (419  mA/)  is  not  only  essential 
for  preventing  polyspermy  and  hardening  the  FE,  but  also 
for  the  normal  embryonic  development  of  the  sea  urchins 
Arbacia  pnnctiilata  and  Strongylocentrotus  purpwatiis 
(Schuel  ct  ai.  1982). 

To  better  understand  the  mechanism  of  FE  elevation— 
an  important  early  event  of  embryonic  development— 
we  concentrated  on  the  effect  of  Na+  deficiency  on  the 
formation  of  the  FE.  In  the  present  study,  the  elevation 
and  morphology  of  FEs  from  normal  and  low  sodium  (2 
mA/)  seawaters  were  investigated.  Envelopes  were  ob- 
served with  phase,  scanning,  and  transmission  electron 
microscopy.  Total  soluble  secreted  protein  and  the  en- 
zymatic activities  of  ovoperoxidase  (Foerder  and  Shapiro. 
1977;  Hall,  1 978)  and  ^-l,3-glucanase( Schuel  et  ai.  1972; 
Wessel  et  ai.  1987)  in  the  fertilization  products  (FP)  were 
compared.  Portions  of  this  investigation  have  been  pre- 
sented in  a  preliminary  form  (Cheng  et  ai.  1989). 

Materials  and  Methods 

Handling  of  gametes 

Sea  urchin  (5.  piirpuratits)  eggs  were  collected  and  acid 
dejellied  as  described  previously  (Green  et  ai.  1990).  Eggs 
were  divided  into  four  equal  portions,  the  three  low  Na+ 
groups:  KC1-,  Tris-,  and  ChCl-substituted  seawaters  (SW) 
and  the  control:  normal  Na+-SW  group.  They  were 
washed  and  incubated  in  the  appropriate  SW  for  1 5-40 
min.  All  seawater  formulations  were  based  on  Cavanaugh 
(1956).  For  the  low  Na+-SWs,  equimolar  concentrations 
of  KG,  Tris  HC1,  or  ChCl  (Sigma)  replaced  NaCl,  yielding 
a  calculated  residual  [Na+]  of  approximately  2  mA/.  Nor- 
mal and  low  Na+  SWs  were  buffered  with  10  mA/  TAPS 
(Sigma)  and  adjusted  to  pH  8.3  with  NaOH  or  KOH, 
respectively.  To  avoid  contamination  from  sperm  proteins 
and  secretions,  eggs  were  activated  by  adding  the  Ca'+ 
ionophore  A23187  (Chambers  et  ai,  1974;  Steinhardt  and 
Epel,  1974)  to  a  final  concentration  of  38  ^Af  in  1%  di- 
methyl sulfoxide  (DMSO).  Experiments  were  performed 
at  20°C. 

Light  microscopy 

Eggs  activated  in  normal  and  low  Na+-SWs  were  ob- 
served continuously  on  a  1  X  3  inch  microscope  slide 
under  a  coverslip  supported  by  a  ring  of  petroleum  jelly. 
Photographs  were  taken  with  phase  contrast  optics  on 
Kodak  Technical  Pan  Film  2415. 

Ovoperoxidase  localization 

Activated  eggs  of  the  control  group  (normal  Na4-SW) 
were  rinsed  at  75  min  postactivation  with  0.45  A/  NaCl- 


0.1  AI  Tris  HC1  (KlebanoftV/  ai.  1979).  Ten  percent  egg 
suspensions  (v/v)  were  made  and  incubated  in  5.6  mA/ 
3,3-diaminobenzidine  (DAB;  Sigma)  in  0.45  A/NaCl-0. 1 
M  Tris  HC1  in  an  ice  bath  for  10  min.  The  experimental 
eggs  (low  Na+)  were  handled  identically  except  that  NaCl 
was  replaced  with  ChCl.  Activated  eggs  were  then  fixed 
and  prepared  for  TEM  as  described  below. 

Transmission  electron  microscopy 

Eggs  were  fixed  with  2%  glutaraldehyde  in  the  appro- 
priate seawaters  for  1  h  at  room  temperature  and  washed 
with  0. 1  A/ sodium  cacodylate  (pH  7.4).  Postfixation  with 
1%  osmium  tetroxide  (OsO4)  was  performed  for  30  min. 
Eggs  were  washed  with  double  distilled  water  and  dehy- 
drated in  ascending  concentrations  of  ethanol.  Ethanol 
was  replaced  with  propylene  oxide  and  eggs  were  infil- 
trated with  EM  bed-812  (Electron  Microscopy  Sciences). 
The  blocks  were  cured  at  58-60°C  for  3  days.  Sections 
were  cut  with  glass  knives  on  a  Reichert-Jung  Ultracut  E; 
mounted  on  copper  grids;  stained  with  lead  citrate  (Reyn- 
olds, 1963)  and  uranyl  acetate;  and  observed  with  a  Philips 
301  transmission  electron  microscope  at  60  kV. 

Scanning  electron  microscopy 

Fixations  were  accomplished  at  20°C  by  mixing  equal 
volumes  of  eggs  with  4%  glutaraldehyde  in  seawater  (with 
10  mMTAPS  pH  8.3)  before  activation  and  at  1,  3,  10, 
30,  and  60  min  postactivation.  Eggs  were  fixed  for  1  h 
and  washed  in  0.1  M  sodium  cacodylate  (pH  7.4).  Post- 
fixation  took  place  in  1%  OsO4  in  0.1  M  sodium  caco- 
dylate (pH  7.4)  for  30  min.  They  were  then  washed  with 
double  distilled  water  and  dehydrated  in  an  ascending 
series  of  ethanol.  Absolute  ethanol  was  replaced  gradually 
by  acetone.  Eggs  were  transferred  to  porous  containers 
(Bio-Rad)  and  processed  for  CO:  critical  point  drying 
(Samdri-790,  Tousimis  Research  Corp.).  The  eggs  were 
then  attached  to  aluminum  mounts  coated  with  colloidal 
silver  liquid  (Ted  Pella,  Inc.)  for  sputter  coating  with  gold: 
palladium  (60:40;  Electron  Microscopy  Sciences)  for  3 
min  in  a  Hummer  VI  (Technics).  Eggs  were  observed  with 
a  JEOL  JSM-35CF  scanning  electron  microscope  at  25 
kV  and  a  condenser  lens  setting  of  3.  The  photos  were 
taken  with  Polaroid  type  55  (4  X  5  in.)  positive/negative 
instant  sheet  film. 

Total  protein  assay 

lonophore-activated  eggs  were  allowed  to  settle  for  ap- 
proximately 10  min  and  the  supernatant  (secreted  FP) 
was  collected  and  centrifuged  by  hand  to  remove  the  few 
remaining  eggs.  Ionophore  activation  resulted  in  at  least 
95%  elevated  FEs.  For  protein  determination  the  proteins 
in  1 .0  ml  of  FP  (5%  egg  suspension,  v/v)  were  precipitated 


348 


S.-D.  CHENG  ET  AL 


by  adding  1 10  n\  ice-cold  50%  trichloroacetic  acid  (TCA) 
and  centrifuged  for  20  min  at  8800  X  g  (Eppendorf  cen- 
trifuge 5413)  at  10°C.  Tubes  were  drained  by  inversion 
and  the  protein  pellets  air  dried.  Bovine  serum  albumin 
(BSA;  Sigma)  was  the  standard.  All  the  precipitates  were 
assayed  according  to  Lowry  el  til.  (1951).  As  a  control, 
protein  determinations  were  performed  with  BSA  in  each 
of  the  seawaters  to  ascertain  their  influences,  if  any,  on 
the  Lowry  procedure. 

0-1,3-glucanase  assay 

Glucanase  activity  was  measured  according  to  Green 
and  Summers  ( 1980).  The  FP  was  collected  as  described 
above.  FP  (0.2  ml;  5%  egg  suspension,  v/v).  normal  Na+- 
or  ChCl-substituted  SW  (0.2  ml),  and  laminarin  (0.2  ml 
of  a  2.5  mg/ml  SW  solution)  were  incubated  for  1  h  at 
37°C.  Then  0.2  ml  of  this  mixture,  0.4  ml  enzyme  solution 
(0.4  ml  glucose  oxidase  and  3  mg  horseradish  peroxidase 
in  50  ml  of  25  mM  phosphate  bufTer,  pH  6)  and  0.4  ml 
o-dianisidine  (40  mg  in  50  ml  double  distilled  water)  were 
incubated  for  10  min  at  37 °C.  The  reaction  was  stopped 
by  the  addition  with  rapid  vortexing  of  0.8  ml  4  A'sulfuric 
acid.  Spectrophotometric  readings  were  taken  at  530  nm. 
Controls  lacking  the  substrate  laminarin  were  used  to 
check  for  the  presence  of  glucose  (the  product  of  the  glu- 
canase-laminarin  reaction)  in  the  FP.  Glucose  was  gen- 
erated only  in  the  presence  of  both  the  FP  and  laminarin. 
A  glucose  solution  was  the  standard.  Control  glucose  de- 
terminations were  performed  in  normal  Na+-SW  and 
ChCl-substituted-SW  to  ascertain  the  effects,  if  any,  of 
ChCl  substitution  on  the  glucose  oxidase-peroxidase  re- 
action. All  the  chemicals  for  this  assay  were  purchased 
from  Sigma. 

Ovoperoxidase  assay 

Ovoperoxidase  assays  were  performed  in  1  ml  contain- 
ing 18  mA/guaiacol  (Sigma),  0.3  mA/  H2O:  (Sigma),  and 
10  mM  TAPS  at  pH  8.0  and  20°C  (Deits  et  a/.,  1984). 
The  reaction  was  started  by  adding  enzyme  (in  the  FP), 
and  the  increase  in  absorbency  at  436  nm  was  recorded 
spectrophotometrically  with  a  strip  chart  recorder.  All  re- 
ported values  are  initial  rates,  because  the  reaction  slows 
after  15-30  s.  A  unit  of  Ovoperoxidase  was  defined  as  that 
which  is  required  to  oxidize  1  ^mole  of  guaiacol  per  min 
in  a  1-ml  assay  volume  (Deits  et  ai,  1984). 

FP  including  the  Ovoperoxidase  was  collected  from  a 
5%  (v/v)  suspension  of  activated  eggs.  Ten  minutes  after 
activation,  eggs  were  settled  by  low  speed  centrifugation 
and  the  supernatant  (FP)  was  removed  for  the  assay. 

Statistics 

Statistical  analyses  for  total  protein,  ft- 1 ,3-glucanase  and 
Ovoperoxidase  assays  were  performed  using  the  Student's 
I  test. 


Results 

Observations  with  light  microscopy 

The  elevation  of  FEs  in  normal  and  low  Na+-SWs  are 
compared  in  Figure  1.  Figure  1A-D  depicts  eggs  at  1  min 
postactivation.  Although  difficult  to  quantify  from  the 
photomicrographs  in  the  normal  Na+-SW  control  group 
(Fig.  1  A,  E),  the  FE  appeared  relatively  thin  at  the  end  of 
the  first  minute  and  thickened  with  increasing  time. 
However,  it  appeared  thicker  (more  refractive)  than  the 
low  Na+  groups,  especially  Tris  and  ChCl  (Fig.  1C,  D). 
At  later  time  points,  the  FE  of  the  control  group  remained 
more  refractive  than  those  of  the  low  Na+  groups.  These 
differences  were  more  striking  at  3  min  postactivation 
when  the  FE  of  ChCl  eggs  began  to  shrink  and  some  col- 
lapsed, while  those  of  the  control  remained  spherical.  By 
30  min  postactivation,  the  FEs  remained  robust  in  the 
normal  and  K+-substituted  SWs  (Fig.  IE,  F),  while  those 
of  the  Tris-  and  choline-substituted  SWs  had  collapsed 
back  nearer  to  the  egg  surface  (Fig.  1G,  H). 

An  interesting  attribute  of  the  activated  eggs  is  that  of 
their  increased  stickiness  in  the  Tris  and  ChCl  groups.  In 
the  first  3  min  there  was  no  apparent  difference  among 
the  4  groups.  At  approximately  4  min  postactivation, 
however,  the  eggs  of  the  Tris  and  ChCl  groups  formed 
extensive  clumps. 

Ultrastructural  changes 

Unactivated  eggs  incubated  in  normal  Na+-  or  ChCl- 
substituted-SWs  and  observed  by  SEM  displayed  similar 
surface  morphology  (Fig.  2A,  E).  At  1  min  postactivation, 
the  FEs  in  both  SWs  were  elevated  with  rounded  (I-form) 
microvillar  projections  (Fig.  2B,  F).  In  contrast  to  FEs  in 
ChCl-substituted  SW  (Fig.  2G),  the  typical  I-T  ("Igloo- 
Tent")  transformation  of  S.  purpuratus  FEs  in  normal 
Na+-SW  was  completed  by  3  min  (Fig.  2C)  and  resembled 
those  of  later  time  points  (Fig.  2D).  However,  ChCl  FEs 
did  not  undergo  the  transformation  even  by  30  min 
(Fig.  2H). 

As  judged  with  TEM  (Fig.  3),  FEs  that  elevated  in  nor- 
mal Na '  -SW  resulted  in  the  well-defined,  angular  T-form 
projections  (Fig.  3A)  characteristic  of  this  species.  How- 
ever, those  in  K'-substituted-SW  appeared  to  be  inter- 
mediate in  form  (Fig.  3B),  compared  to  those  in  Tris-  and 
ChCl-substituted  SWs,  which  were  similar  to  each  other 
in  retaining  rounded  projections  (Fig.  3C,  D). 

The  above  observations  of  the  "soft"  FEs  (incomplete 
formation)  suggested  that  their  permeability,  as  well  as 
their  morphology,  might  be  altered.  Therefore,  several 
measurements  of  permeability  were  undertaken. 

Total  protein  secretion 

Soluble  secreted  protein  that  leaked  through  the  FEs 
of  eggs  activated  in  normal  or  low  Na+  SWs  was  measured 


FERTILIZATION  ENVELOPE  ASSEMBLY 


349 


Figure  1.  Phase  microscopy  of  Strongylocentrotus  piirpiiranix  eggs  activated  in  normal  (A.  E)  and  Na* 
depleted  SWs  (B-D  and  F-H).  A.  E:  Normal  Na+-SW.  B,  F:  KCl-substituted-SW.  C,  G:  Tns-substituted- 
SW.  D,  H:  ChCl-substituted-SW.  A-D:  1  min  postactivation.  E-H:  30  min.  These  micrographs  were  taken 
focusing  on  the  fertilization  envelopes,  fe  =  fertilization  envelope;  PVS  =  perivitelline  space.  Scale  bar 
=  50  ^m. 


(Fig.  4).  FPs  were  collected  at  10  min  postactivation  from 
eggs  pooled  from  several  females.  The  FPs  of  the  normal 
and  K+  -substituted  SW  eggs  had  22.7  ±  1.8  Mg  and  22.2 
±  5.7  Mg  (Mean  ±  S.E.M.)  of  protein/ml  FP,  respectively. 
They  were  not  significantly  different.  However,  the  FPs 
of  Tris-  and  ChCl-substituted  SWs  contained  162.  1  ±  25.9 
Mg  and  168.7  ±  3.4  ^g-  respectively.  This  7-  to  8-fold  in- 
crease over  normal  and  KC1  was  highly  significant  (P 
<  0.0001). 

Control  assays  demonstrated  no  significant  difference 
(95%  confidence  level)  in  TCA-precipitable  BSA  between 
normal  and  low  Na+  SWs.  Therefore,  the  various  SWs 
had  no  adverse  effects  on  the  Lowry  assay.  In  addition, 
supernatant  protein  from  unactivated  eggs  in  DMSO  was 
measured  and  found  to  contribute  little  to  the  total  (  ~  1  .4 
h  see  also  Green  et  ai,  1990). 


ft-  1  ,3-glucanase  secretion 

Glucanase  activity  was  measured  (in  normal  and  ChCl 
SWs)  by  the  amount  of  glucose  hydrolyzed  from  the  /8- 
1,3-glucan  polysaccharide  laminarin  by  egg-derived-glu- 
canase  in  the  FP  (Fig.  5).  Aliquots  of  FP  of  experimental 
and  control  groups  were  taken  at  10  min  postactivation. 
The  glucose  measurements  from  ChCl  and  normal  SW 
were  1.62  ±  0.19  and  0.93  ±0.19  ^moles  glucose  per  ml 
of  FP,  respectively.  Approximately  75%  more  glucanase 


activity  was  found  in  the  FP  from  the  ChCl  eggs.  This 
difference  was  significant  (P  <  0.05). 

Control  incubations  of  glucose  were  assayed  in  normal 
and  ChCl-substituted  SWs,  and  no  significant  differences 
(95%  confidence  level)  were  observed.  Therefore,  it  is  un- 
likely that  the  ChCl  interfered  with  the  glucose  determi- 
nation. 

Ovoperoxidase  secretion 

Ovoperoxidase  released  from  ChCl-SW  eggs  (3.72 
±  0.78  ^moles  guaiacol  oxidized/min/ml  of  FP)  had  sig- 
nificantly higher  activity  (see  Fig.  6)  than  that  released 
from  normal  SW  eggs  (0.82  ±  0.21  jumole/min/ml).  Per- 
oxidase  activities  in  KC1-SW  ( 1 .09  ±  0.30  /Ljmoles/min/ 
ml)  and  Tris-SW  ( 1 .68  ±  0.30  ^moles/min/ml)  FPs  were 
intermediate  between  control  and  ChCl  groups.  There 
were  significant  differences  between  normal  and  ChCl  (P 
<  0.001),  and  Tris  (P  <  0.05).  However,  Ovoperoxidase 
release  was  not  significantly  different  between  normal  and 
KC1(P>0.35). 

Ovoperoxidase  localization 

Because  Ovoperoxidase  is  incorporated  into  the  FE 
(Somers  et  ai,  1989)  and  more  enzyme  activity  was  ob- 
served in  the  FP  of  the  low  Na+  treatments  (see  above), 
it  is  possible  that  the  higher  activity  was  not  only  due  to 


350 


S.-D.  CHENG  ET  AL. 


Figure  2.  SEM  of  Strongylocentrotus purpwatus  egg  surfaces.  A.  VL  ofegg(unactivatedl  in  normal  Na*- 
SW.  B-D.  FEs  of  normal  Na+-SW  eggs  at  1,3,  and  60  min  postactivation.  E.  VL  of  egg  (unactivated)  in 
ChCl-substituted-SW.  F-H.  FEs  of  ChCl-suhstituted-SW  eggs  at  1,3,  and  30  min.  The  microvillar  projections 
of  the  ChCl-suhstituted-SW  eggs  did  not  undergo  the  I-T  transformation.  Scale  bar  =  1  ^m. 


FE  permeability,  but  that  the  ovoperoxidase  was  not  in- 
corporated efficiently  into  the  structure  of  the  FE.  There- 
fore, DAB  localization  of  ovoperoxidase  was  performed. 
Comparing  the  TEM  micrographs  (Fig.  7)  of  FEs  after 
DAB  incubation,  the  normal  SW  FE  (Fig.  7C)  is  con- 
spicuously darker  than  that  of  the  ChCl  FE  (Fig.  7D). 
Although  both  normal  and  ChCl  FEs  stained  more  inten- 
sively than  the  controls  (Fig.  7A,  B),  the  intensity  of  stain- 
ing was  higher  in  the  normal  FEs.  Presumably,  there  was 
more  ovoperoxidase  incorporated  into  the  FEs  in  normal 
Na+-SW  than  in  ChCl-substituted  SW. 

Discussion 

Sea  urchin  eggs  are  excellent  material  for  many  bio- 
logical studies  because  they  can  be  harvested  in  large 
numbers,  cultured  in  a  well-defined  medium  (artificial 
seawater),  and  they  develop  synchronously.  They  are  well 
suited  for  the  study  of  extracellular  self-assembly  (Kay 
and  Shapiro,  1985;  Somers  and  Shapiro,  1989;  Shapiro 
el  al.,  1989).  The  complexity  of  the  transition  of  the  vi- 
telline  layer  (VL)  glycoprotein  to  the  FE  tempts  one  to 
try  to  dissect  the  myriad  of  sequential  processes  involved. 
The  VL  is  not  merely  an  inert  cell  coat,  but  it  serves  as  a 
template  or  scaffolding  upon  which  other  proteins  are  as- 


sembled and  intercalated  under  the  influence  of  several 
enzymes.  A  requisite  for  proper  structuralization  is  the 
presence  of  several  ions  in  the  seawater,  e.g..  Ca2+,  Mg:4 
(Carroll  and  Endress,  1982),  Cl~  (Lynn  et  al.  1988;  Green 
et  al..  1990),  and  Na+  (Schuel  et  al.  1982).  In  the  present 
study,  we  focused  on  the  effects  of  Na+ -depletion  on  the 
elevation  and  structuralization  of  the  FE. 

There  is  some  information  on  the  sea  urchin  egg  during 
fertilization  in  Na+-depleted  seawater.  It  was  found  that 
the  fast  block  to  polyspermy  decayed  concurrently  with 
the  retardation  of  the  depolarization  of  the  egg  plasma 
membrane,  a  Na+-dependent  process  (Jaffe,  1980;  Schuel 
and  Schuel,  1981).  Additional  investigations  have  shown 
that  Na+  accounts  for  the  release  of  acid  from  the  egg, 
resulting  in  an  increased  intracellular  pH.  This  increase 
is  necessary  for  increased  protein  synthesis,  DNA  synthe- 
sis, and  cell  division  (Nishioka  and  Cross,  1978).  In  re- 
lation to  the  assembly  of  the  FE,  Schuel  et  al.  (1982)  dem- 
onstrated that  low  Na+  (19  mM),  ChCl-substituted  sea- 
water  resulted  in  FEs  that  collapsed  and  failed  to  undergo 
normal  structuralization.  including  the  I  to  T  transfor- 
mation of  the  microvillar  casts.  The  inhibition  of  the  nor- 
mal hardening  process  was  attributed  to  the  failure  of  CG 
structural  proteins  to  impregnate  or  insert  into  the  VL. 
However,  this  impairment  of  the  hardening  process  is  dis- 


FERTILIZATION  ENVELOPE  ASSEMBLY 

2.00-1 


1.5O- 


1.OO- 


O.5O- 


351 


Figure  3.  TEM  of  Strongylocentrotus  piirpuraius  FEs.  Eggs  were  fixed 
15  min  postactivation  in  the  following  SWs:  A.  normal  Na+.  B.  K.C1.  C. 
Tris.  D.  ChCl.  Scale  bar  =  0.5  urn. 


tinct  from  the  phenomenon  of  "cross-linking,"  in  that  the 
latter  is  assayed  by  the  disruption  of  FEs  in  urea.  In  their 
experiments,  cross-linking  was  not  affected.  Furthermore, 
K+  and  Li+  substituted  for  Na+  in  normal  structuraliza- 
tion,  while  ChCl  and  Tris  did  not. 


o.oo 


MBL  ChCl 

sea   water   treatment 

Mean   +/-    S.EM 


Figure  5.  0-1,3-glucanase  secretion.  Glucanase  activity  released 
through  FEs  was  assayed  as  described  in  Materials  and  Methods.  The 
Mean  +  S.E.M.  are  shown  for  four  trials.  The  *  denotes  a  statistically 
significant  difference  from  the  control,  normal  SW. 


In  the  present  study  we  lowered  the  Na+  concentration 
to  approximately  2  m.M  and  observed  an  earlier  collapse 
of  the  FE,  3  min  as  opposed  to  30  min.  It  is  not  surprising 
that  our  FEs  collapsed  earlier  than  those  of  Schuel  et  al. 
( 1982).  because  our  Na+  concentration  was  ten-fold  less. 
Additional  evidence  of  the  failure  of  the  sodium-deficient 
FEs  to  harden  is  that  initially  they  expanded  more  than 
the  normal  FEs.  This  greater  distension  may  also  be  re- 
lated to  the  failure  of  proteins  to  insert  into  the  FE,  thereby 
raising  the  hydrostatic  pressure  in  the  perivitelline  space 
(Schuel  et  al.,  1974;  Green  and  Summers,  1980).  However, 
within  a  few  minutes,  the  FE  began  to  shrink,  suggesting 
that  the  FE  was  permeable  to  the  secreted  proteins,  and 
this  allowed  for  a  decrease  in  the  hydrostatic  pressure 


200- 


150- 


<U      1OO 

O 

Q 


Dl 


5O- 


MBL  KCI  Tris  ChCl 

sea    water    treatment 

Mean   +/-    S.EM. 

Figure  4.  Protein  release  through  FEs.  Protein  concentrations  were 
determined  by  the  Lowry  assay.  Each  measurement  represents  the  Mean 
±  S.E.M.  of  three  trials.  The  *  denotes  a  statistically  significant  difference 
from  the  control,  normal  SW. 


5-1 


3- 


MBL  KCI  Tris  ChCl 

sea  water    treatment 

Mean   +/-   S.E.M. 

Figure  6.  Ovoperoxidase  secretion.  Ovoperoxidase  activity  released 
through  FEs  was  measured  as  described  in  Materials  and  Methods.  Mean 
±  S.E.M.  are  shown  (n  =  3-7)  and  *  denotes  statistically  significant 
differences  from  the  control,  normal  SW. 


352 


S.-D.  CHENG  AT  AL. 


^»; 

.       .  -  -  >,V- 

L.-    .  •  *L  >-;• 


*  <yv*»  •        •  ' 

•• 

! 


;- 
7C 


Figure  7.  Ultrastructural  localization  of  ovoperoxidase.  The  pointed  T-form  microvillar  projections  on 
normal  Na*-FE  (15  min  after  activation)  and  the  rounded  I-form  microvillar  projections  on  ChCl-FE  are 
demonstrated  (A  and  B,  respectively).  After  DAB  localization  of  ovoperoxidase  the  FEs  (C,  normal  Na+;  D, 
ChCl)  stained  more  intensely  than  those  of  the  controls  (A  and  B)  in  both  normal  Na*  and  ChCl-substituted- 
SW.  Furthermore,  the  staining  of  the  normal  Na*  was  darker  than  that  of  the  ChCl.  This  suggests  that  less 
ovoperoxidase  was  incorporated  into  the  FE  of  eggs  activated  in  ChCl-suhstituted  SW.  D  is  a  montage 
showing  the  FE  and  cortex  of  the  same  egg.  Scale  bar  =  0.5  //m. 


FERTILIZATION  ENVELOPE  ASSEMBLY 


353 


within  the  perivitelline  space.  This  conclusion  is  strength- 
ened by  the  fact  that  we  observed  a  greater  (approx.  eight- 
fold) increase  in  total  proteins  secreted  into  the  ambient 
seawater  in  Tris  and  ChCl  seawaters.  Glucanase  and  ovo- 
peroxidase  secretions  also  increased  significantly.  Because 
ovoperoxidase  is  normally  incorporated  into  the  FE 
(Somers  and  Shapiro,  1989).  the  increase  in  soluble  ovo- 
peroxidase activity  is  probably  related  to  both  increased 
permeability  and  decreased  incorporation  of  this  enzyme 
into  the  nascent  FE.  The  substitution  of  K+  for  Na+  did 
not  significantly  interfere  with  hardening  (structuraliza- 
tion),  nor  did  it  change  protein  or  ovoperoxidase  secretion 
significantly.  This  observation  is  consistent  with  the  nor- 
mal FE  formation  in  K+-  or  Li '-substituted  seawaters 
(Schudetal..  1982). 

The  results  reported  herein  are  similar  to  those  obtained 
inCl  -deficient  seawaters  (Lynn  el  al..  1988;  Green  el  a/.. 
1990).  Moreover,  our  results  are  strikingly  similar  to  those 
of  Battaglia  and  Shapiro  (1988).  who  reported  similar 
findings  when  they  inhibited  egg  surface  transglutaminase 
activity  with  primary  amines.  This  similarity  suggests  that 
both  Cl  and  Na+  may  be  important  for  the  transgluta- 
minase-catalyzed  early  cross-linking  that  occurs  before  the 
ovoperoxidase-catalyzed  cross-linking.  However,  we  can 
not  ignore  the  possibility  that  the  observed  effects  may  be 
the  result  of  ChCl  or  Tris  addition,  rather  then  exclusion 
of  Na+.  This  is  a  drawback  to  any  substitution  experiment. 
The  size  of  the  substituted  ionic  species  may  be  important 
because  K+  and  Li+  are  closer  in  size  to  Na+  than  are 
either  Tris  or  ChCl.  This  question,  too.  remains  to  be 
resolved. 

Another  interesting  observation  of  these  experiments 
was  the  apparent  paucity  of  hyalin  in  the  perivitelline 
space  of  eggs  activated  in  ChCl-substituted-SW  (e.g..  Figs. 
3A,  D  and  7A,  B).  This  observation  and  the  increased 
glucanase  activity  in  the  ambient  sodium-deficient  sea- 
water  may  be  related  to  the  possibility  that  glucanase  per- 
forms its  major  function  on  the  hyaline  layer  of  the  egg 
(Wessel  el  al..  1987)  in  a  Na+-dependent  manner.  This 
possibility  awaits  further  experimentation. 

Our  results  demonstrate  that  the  ionic  composition  of 
the  seawater  significantly  influences  the  formation  of  the 
FE  in  the  sea  urchin  51.  purpnrutus.  These  results  are  con- 
sistent with  other  published  reports.  Moreover,  the  phe- 
nomenon of  regulated  extracellular  assembly  remains  an 
intriguing  field  of  study,  to  which  the  study  of  sea  urchin 
FE  formation  can  contribute. 

Acknowledgments 

The  authors  wish  to  thank  Drs.  Frank  N.  Low  and 
Joseph  B.  Delcarpio  for  their  SEM  technical  assistance, 
and  Drs.  William  J.  Swartz  and  John  W.  Lynn  for  their 
suggestions  regarding  this  paper.  This  work  was  supported 


by  an  Edward  G.  Schlieder  Educational  Foundation  Grant 
toJ.D.G. 

Literature  Cited 

Battaglia,  D.  E.,  and  B.  M.  Shapiro.  1988.  Hierarchies  of  protein  cross- 
linking  in  the  extracellular  matrix:  involvement  of  an  egg  surface 
transglutaminase  in  early  stages  of  fertilization  envelope  assembly. 
J.  Cell  Biol.  107:  2447-2454. 

Carroll,  E.  J.,  and  A.  G.  Endress.  1982.  Sea  urchin  fertilization  en- 
velope: uncoupling  of  cortical  granule  exocytosis  from  envelope  as- 
sembly and  isolation  of  an  envelope  intermediate  from  Strongy/o- 
centrotus  purpuratus  embryos.  Dev.  Biol.  94:  252-258. 

Cavanaugh,  G.  M.,  ed.  1956.  Pp.  67-69  in  Formulae  and  Methods  of 
the  Marine  Biological  Lahoralory  Chemical  Room.  Marine  Biological 
Laboratory,  Woods  Hole.  MA. 

Chambers,  E.  L.,  B.  C.  Pressman,  and  B.  Rose.  1974.  The  activation 
of  sea  urchin  eggs  by  the  divalent  ionophores  A23187  and  X-537A. 
Kioehem  Btoi'hys.  Res  Coinni.  60:  126-132. 

Cheng,  S.-D.,  P.  S.  Glas,  and  J.  D.  Green.  1989.  Enzymatic  and  mor- 
phological analysis  of  fertilization  envelope  (FE)  assembly  in  low 
Na+  sea  water.  J  Cell  Biol  109:  128a. 

Deits,  T.,  M.  Farrance,  E.  S.  Kay,  L.  Medill,  E.  E.  Turner,  P.  J.  Weidman, 
and  B.  M.  Shapiro.  1984.  Purification  and  properties  of  ovoperox- 
idase. the  enzyme  responsible  for  hardening  the  fertilization  mem- 
brane of  the  sea  urchin  egg.  J.  Biol  Client.  21:  13,525-13.533. 

Foerder,  C.  A.,  and  B.  M.  Shapiro.  1977.  Release  of  ovoperoxidase 
from  sea  urchin  eggs  hardens  the  fertilization  membrane  with  tyrosine 
crosslinks.  Proc.  Nail.  Acaci  Sci.  USA  14:  4214-4218. 

Green,  J.  D.,  and  R.  G.  Summers.  1980.  Formation  of  the  cortical 
concavity  at  fertilization  in  the  sea  urchin  egg.  DOT  Growth  Differ 
22(6):  821-829. 

Green,  J.  D.,  P.  S.  Glas,  S.-D.  Cheng,  and  J.  VV.  Lynn. 
1990.  Fertilization  envelope  assembly  in  sea  urchin  eggs  inseminated 
in  chloride-deficient  sea  water:  II.  Biochemical  effects.  Mol.  Reprod. 
DOT  25:  177-185. 

Hall,  II.  G.  1978.  Hardening  of  the  sea  urchin  fertilization  envelope 
by  peroxidase-catalyzed  phenolic  coupling  of  tyrosines.  Cell  15:  343- 
355. 

Jaffe,  L.  A.  1980.  Electrical  polyspermy  block  in  sea  urchin  eggs:  nic- 
otine and  low  sodium  experiments.  DOT'.  Growth  Differ.  22:  503- 
507. 

Kay,  E.  S.,  and  B.  M.  Shapiro.  1985.  The  formation  of  the  fertilization 
membrane  of  the  sea  urchin  egg.  Pp.  45-80  in  Biology  of  Fertilization, 
\'ol  3.  The  Fertilisation  Response  of  the  Egg.  C.  B.  Metz  and  A. 
Monroy,  eds.  Academic  Press.  Orlando. 

Klebanoff,  S.  J.,  C.  A.  Foerder,  M.  Eddy,  and  B.  M.  Shapiro. 
1979.  Metabolic  similarities  between  fertilization  and  phagocytosis. 
J.  Exp.  Med.  149:  938-953. 

Lowry,  O.  H.,  N.  J.  Rosebrough,  A.  L.  Farr,  and  R.  J.  Randall. 
1951.  Protein  measurement  with  the  folin  phenol  reagent.  J.  Biol. 
Chem.  193:  265-275. 

Lynn,  J.  VV.,  R.  L.  Goddard,  P.  Glas,  and  J.  D.  Green. 
1988.  Fertilization  envelope  assembly  in  sea  urchin  eggs  inseminated 
in  Cr-deficient  sea  water:  I.  Morphological  effects.  Gamete  Res.  21: 
135-149. 

Nishioka,  D.,  and  N.  Cross.  1978.  The  role  of  external  sodium  in  sea 
urchin  fertilization.  Pp.  403-413  in  Cell  Reproduction:  In  Honor  of 
D  Mazia.  E.  R.  Dirksen,  D.  M.  Prescott,  and  C.  F.  Fox,  eds.  Academic 
Press.  New  York. 

Reynolds,  E.  S.  1963.  The  use  of  lead  citrate  at  high  pH  as  an  electron 
opaque  stain  in  electron  microscopy.  J.  Cell  Biol.  17:  208-212. 

Schuel,  H.,  W.  L.  Wilson,  R.  S.  Bressler,  J.  W.  Kelly,  and  J.  R.  Wilson. 
1972.  Purification  of  cortical  granules  from  unfertilized  sea  urchin 
egg  homogenates  by  zonal  centnfugation.  Dev.  Biol.  29:  307-320. 


354 


S.-D.  CHENG  ET  AL. 


Schuel,  H.,  J.  \V.  Kelly,  E.  R.  Berger,  and  W.  L.  Wilson.  1974.  Sulfated 
acid  mucopolysacchandes  in  the  cortical  granules  of  eggs.  Effects  of  qua- 
ternary  ammonium  salts  on  fertilization.  Exp.  Cell  Re*  88:  24-30. 

Schuel,  H.,  and  R.  Schuel.  1981.  A  rapid  sodium-dependent  block  to 
polyspermy  in  sea  urchin  eggs.  Dev.  Bid/  87:  249-258. 

Schuel,  H.,  R.  Schuel,  P.  Dandekar,  J.  Boldt,  and  R.  G.  Summers. 
1982.  Sodium  requirements  in  hardening  of  the  fertilization  en- 
velope and  embryonic  development  in  sea  urchins.  Bio/.  Bull  162: 
202-213. 

Shapiro,  B.  M.,  C.  E.  Somers,  and  P.  J.  \\eidman.  1989.  Extracellular 
remodeling  during  fertilization.  Pp.  25 1-276  in  The  Cell  Biology  of 
Fertilization,  H.  Schatten  and  G.  Schatten.  eds.  Academic  Press.  San 
Diego. 

Somers,  C.  E.,  D.  E.  Battaglia,  and  B.  M.  Shapiro.  1989.  Localization 
and  developmental  fate  of  ovoperoxidase  and  proteoliaisin,  two  pro- 


teins involved  in  fertilization  envelope  assembly.  Dev.  Biol.  131:  226- 

235. 
Somers,  C.  E.,  and  B.  M.  Shapiro.  1989.     Insights  into  the  molecular 

mechanisms  involved  in  sea  urchin  fertilization  envelope  assembly. 

Dev.  Growth  Difj.  31(1):  1-7. 
Steinhardt,  R.  A.,  and  D.  Epel.  1974.     Activation  of  sea  urchin  eggs 

by  calcium   lonophore.   Proc.   Nail.  Acad.   Sci.    I'SA   71:    1915- 

1919. 
Weidman,  P.  J.,  E.  S.  Kay,  and  B.  M.  Shapiro.  1985.     Assembly  of  the 

sea  urchin  fertilization  membrane:  isolation  of  proteoliaisin.  a  cal- 
cium-dependent ovoperoxidase  binding  protein.  J.  Cell  Biol.  100: 

938-946. 
Wessel,  G.  M.,  M.  R.  Truschel,  S.  A.  Chambers,  and  D.  R.  McClay. 

1987.     A  cortical  granule-specific  enzyme,  /i-l,3-glucanase,  in  sea 

urchin  eggs.  Gamete  Res  18:  339-348. 


Reference:  Biol  Bull.  180:  355-371.  (June.  1991) 


Embryonic  Development  of  the  American  Lobster 

(Homams  americanus):  Quantitative  Staging  and 

Characterization  of  an  Embryonic  Molt  Cycle 

S.  M.   HELLUY  AND  B.  S.  BELTZ 
Department  of  Biological  Sciences.  Wellesley  College,  Wellesley,  Massachusetts  02181 


Abstract.  The  growth  of  a  single  brood  of  lobsters 
(Homarus  americanus  Milne-Edwards  1837)  maintained 
at  constant  temperature  is  studied  from  the  naupliar  stage 
to  hatching,  and  the  sequence  of  appearance  of  morpho- 
logical, anatomical,  and  behavioral  characteristics  ob- 
served. A  percent-staging  system  based  upon  Perkins'  eye 
index  (1972)  is  presented,  and  ten  equally  spaced  embry- 
onic stages  are  illustrated  and  characterized  at  different 
levels  of  resolution:  whole  eggs,  dissected  embryos,  an- 
tennulae  and  telsons.  The  tegumentary  and  setal  changes 
in  the  telson  show  that  a  complete  molt  cycle  takes  place 
in  the  egg  starting  at  about  12%  embryonic  development 
(El 2%)  with  the  molt  of  the  nauplius  into  the  metanau- 
plius  and  ending  just  after  hatching  when  the  metanau- 
plius  molts  into  a  first  stage  larva  (LI,  first  zoea).  At  E30%, 
the  cuticle  begins  to  separate  from  the  setae  in  the  telson; 
this  signals  the  start  of  Orach's  (1939)  stage  D0  of  the 
metanaupliar  embryonic  molt  cycle.  At  that  time,  the  first 
sign  of  organogenesis  of  the  LI,  the  formation  of  the  en- 
dopod  of  the  antennulae,  becomes  visible;  presumed  sen- 
sory neurons  and  their  axons  are  observed  at  the  tip  of 
the  exopod  of  the  antennulae  where  a  giant  sensillum  is 
differentiating.  During  D0  the  setae  of  the  first  larval  stage 
are  forming  proximally  and  medially  in  the  bilobed  telson 
under  the  metanaupliar  cuticle.  At  E90%,  these  setae  are 
retracting,  and  the  embryo  has  entered  stage  D,.  After 
hatching  (£100%),  the  telson  of  the  free  metanauplius 
(prelarva)  shows  the  characteristics  of  stage  D:_3  and  ec- 
dysis  soon  follows.  The  arrested  development  observed  at 
constant  temperature  in  the  experimental  brood  occurred 
at  stage  D0  of  the  metanaupliar  molt  cycle,  whereas  de- 
velopment was  resumed  as  the  embryos  entered  stage  D, . 
These  changes  in  developmental  pace  from  D0  to  D!  in 
the  embryonic  molt  cycle  are  parallel  to  those  occurring 

Received  3  December  1990;  accepted  8  March  1991. 


in  older  lobsters  (Aiken,  1973).  The  quantitative  staging 
of  lobster  development  from  extrusion  to  hatching,  and 
the  description  of  the  embryonic  molt  cycle  will  facilitate 
future  investigations  on  particular  aspects  of  the  embryo- 
genesis  of  Homarus  such  as  neural  differentiation. 

Introduction 

Studies  on  lobsters  and  other  crustaceans  have  made  a 
significant  contribution  to  our  understanding  of  neural 
organization  and  the  control  of  behavior  (see  Wiese  el  ai. 
1990).  There  is  increasing  interest  in  examining  the  on- 
togenesis of  particular  behaviors  and  the  cellular  archi- 
tecture that  is  the  basis  for  those  behaviors  (Kravitz,  1988; 
Govind.  1989;  Sandeman  and  Sandeman,  1990).  Re- 
search on  neural  development  at  the  embryonic  level  in 
Homarus  is  flourishing  (Cole  and  Lang.  1980;  Beltz  and 
Kravitz,  1987;  Beltz  el  a/..  1990;  Helluy  and  Beltz,  1990; 
Meier  and  Reichert,  1990),  but  progress  has  been  limited 
by  the  lack  of  adequate  documentation  on  the  general 
development  of  this  organism  in  the  egg,  as  well  as  by  the 
absence  of  a  staging  system  for  the  total  embryonic  period. 
These  two  problems  are  addressed  in  this  paper. 

Recent  developmental  studies  in  Homarus  have  dealt 
primarily  with  the  perihatching  period  (Davis,  1964;  En- 
nis,  1975;  Charmantier  and  Aiken,  1987),  and  larval  and 
postlarval  life  (Phillips  and  Sastry,  1980;  Charmantier, 
1987),  whereas  most  of  the  literature  concerned  with  the 
prehatching  period  dates  back  to  the  nineteenth  century 
(Bumpus,  1891;  Herrick,  1895).  The  latter  studies  are  a 
remarkable  achievement  of  patient  and  detailed  obser- 
vation and  are  illustrated  by  elegant  drawings  (Herrick, 
1895),  but  the  modern  microscopic  and  photographic 
methods  used  in  this  study  are  necessary  to  provide  added 
resolution.  The  nineteenth  century  studies  also  tend  to 
focus  on  early  embryogenesis  while  providing  little  or  no 
information  about  middle  and  late  development  in  the 


355 


356 


S.  M.  HELLUY  AND  B.  S.  BELTZ 


egg,  and  the  embryonic  molt  cycle.  A  deeper  knowledge 
of  lobster  embryology  could  also  provide  more  insight 
and  understanding  of  studies  that  examine  particular  as- 
pects of  development,  such  as  the  influence  of  temperature 
on  growth  rate  (Templeman.  1940;  Perkins.  1972).  pop- 
ulation dynamics  (Schuur  et  al.  1976;  Hepper  and  Gough, 
1978),  the  chemical  composition  and  calorific  content  of 
the  eggs  (Pandian  1970a,  b;  Sasaki,  1984;  Sasaki  et  al. 
1986),  or  the  differentiation  of  particular  organs  or  sys- 
tems, such  as  heart  and  gut  (Burrage,  1978;  Burrage  and 
Sherman,  1979),  and,  again,  nervous  system. 

The  principal  features  involved  in  the  reproduction  and 
early  development  of  the  lobster  Homarus  americanus 
are  well  known.  After  copulation,  spermatozoa  are  stored 
by  the  female  for  several  months  until  oviposition  and 
fertilization  occur  (Aiken  and  Waddy,  1980).  In  New 
England  waters,  egg  development  spans  about  10  months, 
from  egg  extrusion  in  July  or  August  to  hatching  the  fol- 
lowing May  or  June  (Bumpus,  1891;  Herrick,  1895).  Fol- 
lowing extrusion,  the  eggs  are  carried  on  the  abdomen  of 
the  mother,  attached  to  the  pleopods.  Homarus  has  rel- 
atively large,  telolecithal  eggs.  Superficial  cleavage  leads 
to  the  formation  of  a  blastoderm,  and  the  central  mass 
of  yolk  remains  undivided  (Bumpus.  1891).  After  only  a 
few  days,  the  naupliar  organization  is  apparent.  The  nau- 
plius,  which  is  a  developmental  hallmark  of  crustaceans, 
is  characterized  by  the  presence  of  a  median  eye  and  three 
pairs  of  appendages:  the  antennulae,  antennae,  and  man- 
dibles (Shiino,  1988).  The  metanauplius  arises  from  the 
differentiation  and  growth  of  the  postmandibular  ap- 
pendages. Homarus  hatches  as  a  mature  metanauplius 
(prelarva.  prezoea)  that  molts  rapidly  into  the  first  larval 
stage  (Davis,  1964).  There  are  three  pelagic  larval  stages 
swimming  with  the  feathery  exopodites  of  six  pairs  of  tho- 
racic limbs.  A  metamorphosis  leads  to  the  formation  of 
a  postlarva  (fourth  stage)  with  most  of  the  adult  charac- 
teristics (Charmantier,  1987).  The  postlarva,  which  swims 
in  a  fully  extended  posture  using  its  pleopods.  later  settles 
on  the  substrate.  The  duration  of  larval  life,  in  the  order 
of  a  few  weeks,  depends  largely  on  temperature. 

For  the  present  study,  behavioral,  morphological,  an- 
atomical, and  morphometric  data  were  gathered  from 
whole  eggs  and  dissected  embryos.  A  percent-staging 
scheme  using  the  size  of  the  pigmented  area  in  the  lateral 
eyes  [the  eye  index  (Perkins,  1972)]  was  adopted.  Sub- 
sequently, ten  equally  spaced  developmental  stages  were 
documented  in  detail  with  the  eggs  of  different  females. 
Particular  attention  was  given  to  the  growth  and  differ- 
entiation of  the  antennulae  and  telson.  The  antennulae, 
which  are  lined  with  aesthetascs  (olfactory  sensilla)  in 
postembryonic  animals  from  second  larval  stage  on,  were 
examined  to  gain  insight  into  the  ontogeny  of  the  olfactory 
sensory  apparatus.  The  telson  was  studied  to  elucidate 
how  the  round  bilobed  telson  of  the  embryo  is  transformed 
into  the  triangular  telson  of  the  first  larval  stage. 


Materials  and  Methods 

Lobster  and  egg  maintenance 

Egg-bearing  female  lobsters  Homarus  americanus 
(Crustacea,  Malacostraca,  Decapoda,  Reptantia,  Asta- 
cidea,  Nephropidae)  were  obtained  from  the  Massachu- 
setts State  Lobster  Hatchery  on  Martha's  Vineyard,  Mas- 
sachusetts, and  kept  in  recirculating  artificial  seawater.  In 
addition,  eggs  detached  from  the  mother's  abdomen  were 
provided  by  the  New  England  Aquarium  in  Boston,  Mas- 
sachusetts, where  lobsters  were  reared  in  filtered,  temper- 
ature-controlled seawater.  These  detached  eggs  were 
maintained  in  our  laboratory  in  free-floating  net  enclo- 
sures in  artificial  seawater.  We  found  that  hanging  the 
clumps  of  eggs  with  surgical  thread,  and  allowing  them 
to  float,  led  to  good  survival  rates.  Three  tanks  were 
maintained  at  temperatures  of  10  ±  2°C,  18  ±  2°C.  and 
20  ±  2°C,  to  slow  or  accelerate  the  rate  of  development 
of  the  eggs,  at  a  salinity  between  27  and  32  ppt  in  a  12: 
12  lightdark  cycle. 

The  experimental  brood 

We  have  not  had  any  success  promoting  egg  extrusion 
in  females  held  in  recirculating  tanks,  probably  because 
of  the  variety  of  complex  environmental  factors  necessary 
for  this  event  (Waddy  and  Aiken,  1984).  Therefore,  in 
mid-October,  the  egg-bearing  female  containing  the 
youngest  eggs  was  chosen  from  a  collection  of  approxi- 
mately 200  gravid  females  collected  by  fishermen  for  the 
State  Lobster  Hatchery.  The  earliest  stage  observed  in  the 
experimental  brood  was  a  cleavage  stage.  The  approximate 
date  of  extrusion  was  calculated  as  follows:  in  Temple- 
man's  (1940)  experiments,  the  period  from  the  late  nau- 
plius  to  the  first  appearance  of  pigment  in  the  lateral  eyes 
(26  days)  lasted  about  45%  of  the  time  required  for  the 
development  from  extrusion  to  appearance  of  eye  pigment 
(58  days)  at  12-13°C,  and  41%  (11/27)  in  Herrick's  ex- 
periments (1895,  p.  56)  at  21°C.  In  the  lobster  (Temple- 
man, 1940;  Perkins,  1972)  and  in  insects  (Bentley  et  al., 
1979),  developmental  events  are  more  condensed  or  ex- 
panded in  time  depending  on  temperature,  but  the  pro- 
portion of  the  total  duration  of  embryogenesis  devoted  to 
each  developmental  event  does  not  change  with  temper- 
ature. In  the  present  study  at  18°C,  the  development  from 
late  nauplius  to  the  first  appearance  of  eye  pigment  took 
9  days;  therefore,  by  extrapolation  from  the  data  of  Tem- 
pleman (1940)  and  Herrick  (1895),  the  period  from  ex- 
trusion to  first  appearance  of  eye  pigment  would  be  pre- 
dicted to  last  20-22  days.  Thus,  the  estimated  date  of 
extrusion  was  calculated  to  be  21  days  prior  to  the  ap- 
pearance of  eye  pigment.  Note  that  extrusion  did  occur 
in  the  wild  in  water  at  seasonal  temperatures. 

Observations  were  made  on  the  experimental  brood 
kept  at  18  ±  2°C  for  five  months  (mid-October  to  mid- 


EMBRYONIC  DEVELOPMENT  OF  THE  LOBSTER 


357 


March,  see  Table  I).  The  female  died  in  mid-January, 
when  the  eggs  were  at  66%  development:  she  was  stripped 
of  eggs  and  the  spawn  was  suspended  in  nets  in  the  tank. 
The  eggs  were  agitated  daily  to  try  to  replace  the  vigorous 
beating  of  the  pleopods  of  the  mother.  In  the  experimental 
brood,  the  majority  of  the  eggs  attained  the  hatching  stage 
but  very  few  actually  hatched  into  free  metanaupliae;  still 
fewer  molted  into  first  larval  stages.  Those  larvae  that  did 
emerge  were  perfectly  normal  animals.  The  smoothness 
of  growth  curves  of  the  experimental  brood  (see  Results) 
and  numerous  observations  on  the  progeny  of  other  fe- 
males confirmed  that  the  free  eggs  of  the  experimental 
brood  followed  a  normal  course  of  development  after  the 
death  of  the  mother. 

Five  live  eggs  from  the  experimental  brood  were  ex- 
amined every  two  or  three  days  for  the  first  two  months, 
then  once  a  week  until  hatching.  As  soon  as  the  heart  was 
formed,  the  heart  beat  was  confirmed  in  each  embryo  to 
ensure  that  the  observed  eggs  were  alive.  During  each  ob- 
servation period,  the  width  and  length  of  the  pigmented 
area  in  the  lateral  eyes  (Fig.  1 )  and  the  greatest  axis  of  the 
egg  were  measured  in  intact  eggs:  following  dissection,  the 
length  of  the  cephalothorax  was  measured  ventrally  from 
the  median  eye  to  the  anterior  margin  of  the  abdomen 
(Fig.  2).  Behavioral  observations,  such  as  antennal 
twitching  or  tail  flipping  during  dissection,  were  also 
noted.  Photographs  of  whole  eggs  and  dissected  embryos 
were  taken  with  a  Zeiss  stereomicroscope. 


Developmental  staging  system 

A  developmental  scale  was  designed  that  used  the  eye 
index  (Perkins,  1972)  as  a  marker  of  developmental  pro- 
gress. The  eye  index  is  defined  as  the  average  of  the  length 
and  the  width  of  the  brown  screening  pigment  spot  (in 
micrometers)  in  the  lateral  eyes.  The  first  measurable  eye 
pigment  spot  had  an  eye  index  of  70  ^m  (Perkins,  1972; 
present  study).  Therefore,  development  prior  to  the  ap- 
pearance of  eye  pigment  was  characterized  using  time 
rather  than  the  eye  index.  The  estimated  duration  of  de- 
velopment of  the  experimental  brood  was  159  days  (Table 
I).  Eye  pigment  first  appears  at  13.2%  of  the  total  time 
from  extrusion  to  hatching,  while  the  eye  index  at  first 
appearance  of  pigment  (70  jum)  is  12.2%  of  the  eye  index 
of  the  experimental  brood  at  hatching  (578  /^m)  (Table 
I).  These  values  indicate  that  there  is  little  difference  during 
early  embryogenesis  between  staging  based  on  time  and 
that  based  on  the  eye  index;  time-staging  was  used  prior 
to,  and  eye  index-staging  after  1 5%  development  (Table 
I).  Later  in  embryogenesis,  because  of  the  period  of  de- 
velopmental arrest  (see  Results),  staging  based  upon  time 
is  no  longer  valid;  the  morphometric  marker  (eye  index) 
must  then  be  used. 


Table  I 

Dales  of  observation  ot  the  experimental  brood  ol  Homarus 
amencanus  maintained  at  1S°C.  age.  eye  index,  and 
percent- staging  svstem.  The  dotted  line  signals  the  transition  between 
percent  of  total  lime  from  extrusion  to  hatching  and  percent  ol  eye 
index  at  hatching 


g 

Date 
8-89 

Embryonic 
age  (days) 

Eye  index 
(Mm) 

Stage 
(%) 

1 
1 

1 
1 
1 
] 

0-08 
0-16 
0-18 
0-20 
0-23 
0-25 
0-27 
0-29 

(I 
8 
10 
12 
15 
17 
19 
21 

70.6 

0 
5.3 
6.3 
7.6 
9.4 
10.7 
12.0 
13.2 

0-31 

23 

72.5 

14.5 

1-02 

25 

98.0 

17.0 

1-04 

27 

139.2 

24.1 

1-06 

29 

145.0 

25.1 

1-08 

31 

156.8 

27.1 

1-1  1 

34 

160.7 

27.8 

1-18 

41 

213.6 

37.0 

1-24 

47 

248.9 

43.1 

2-01 

54 

282.2 

48.8 

2-08 

61 

307.7 

53.2 

2-15 

68 

329.3 

57.0 

2-23 

76 

352.8 

61.0 

2-29 

82 

366.5 

63.4 

( 

M-05 

89 

386.1 

66.8 

( 

H-12 

96 

382.2 

66.1 

( 

)1-19 

103 

425.3 

73.6 

1 

H-26 

110 

447.0 

77.3 

32-02 

117 

441.0 

76.3 

J2-09 

124 

458.6 

79.3 

32-16 

131 

466.5 

80.7 

32-23 

138 

474.3 

82.1 

33-02 

145 

474.3 

82.1 

33-09 

152 

542.9 

93.9 

33-16 

159 

578.2 

100.0 

Characterization  often  embryonic  stages  and  oj  the 
embryonic  molt  cycle 

Following  the  adoption  of  the  percent-staging  system, 
eggs  from  different  broods  were  studied  in  more  detail  at 
every  10%  increment  in  development.  The  eye  index  at 
hatching  was  estimated  at  570  ±  20  Mm  (see  Discussion), 
and  therefore  each  10%  increment  in  developmental  ma- 
turity was  characterized  by  an  increase  of  57  /*m  in  the 
eye  index.  The  stage  described  as  10%  was  reached  on 
day  16  in  the  experimental  brood:  the  late  egg-nauplius. 
Eggs  were  also  examined  when  their  eye  indices  measured 
1 14  ±  6  ^m  (E20%),  171  ±  4  Mm  (E30%),  228  ±  7  urn 
(E40%),  285  Mm  ±  14  (E50%),  342  ±  1 1  M™  (E60%),  399 
±  17  ^m  (E70%),  456  ±  5  Mm  (E80%),  513  ±  20  Mm 
(E90%),  570  ±  20  Mm  (£100%).  The  varying  range  for 
each  stage  reflected  embryo  availability  and  limitations 


358 


S.  M.  HELLUY  AND  B.  S.  BELTZ 


imposed  by  the  precision  of  the  ocular  micrometer. 
Twenty  micrometers  represents  3.5%  of  the  total  devel- 
opment scale.  To  characterize  each  of  the  ten  stages  and 
the  early  postembryonic  stages,  the  same  protocol  de- 
scribed earlier  for  the  experimental  brood  was  used.  Pho- 
tographs of  whole  eggs  (Figs.  3,  4)  and  dissected  embryos 
(Fig.  5)  were  taken  with  a  Zeiss  stereomicroscope.  In  ad- 
dition, antennulae  (Fig.  6)  and  telsons  (Fig.  7)  were  also 
severed,  examined  fresh,  and  photographed  using  a  Zeiss 
IM35  photoinvertoscope  equipped  with  modulation  con- 
trast optics  (Hoffman,  1977). 

Drach  (1939)  and  Drach  and  Tchernigovtzeff  (1967) 
designated  the  phases  of  ecdysis  in  crustaceans  by  letters 
from  A  to  E:  A  and  B  are  the  postmolt  periods,  C  the 
intermolt,  D  the  premolt  period,  and  E  the  ecdysis  proper. 
In  the  present  study,  this  system  was  used  to  characterize 
the  molt  cycle  of  Homarus  that  occurs  within  the  egg 
envelopes.  The  period  of  the  embryonic  molt  cycle  was 
determined  by  matching  setal  changes  in  the  telson  of 
embryos  (Figs.  7  and  8)  with  the  setal  changes  in  the  telson 
previously  documented  in  larvae  of  Homarus  americanm 
(Rao  el  al.,  1973;  Sasaki,  1984)  and  in  the  pleopods  in 
juveniles  (Aiken,  1973;  1980)  during  molt  cycles.  The 
subdivisions  of  stage  D  (D0,  D,,  D:_,)  and  their  distin- 
guishing features  are  those  described  by  Sasaki  (1984). 

Terminology 

More  than  70  terms  have  been  used  to  refer  to  the 
various  embryonic  and  larval  stages  of  decapods  (Gore, 
1985).  The  form  that  arises  from  the  differentiation  and 
growth  of  the  postmandibular  appendages  in  the  Amer- 


ican lobster  egg  has  been  called  a  "post-nauplius"  by  Her- 
nck  ( 1 895)  or  "postnauplius"  by  Helluy  and  Beltz  ( 1 990). 
In  the  present  study  "metanauplius"  is  used,  a  term  com- 
monly assigned  to  the  form  developing  just  past  the  nau- 
pliar  stage  (Wear,  1974;  Williamson.  1982;Shiino,  1988). 
The  form  that  is  released  from  the  egg  envelopes  and  rap- 
idly molts  into  the  first  larval  stage  is  usually  called  a 
"prelarva"  or  "prezoea";  however,  the  term  "mature  me- 
tanauplius" seems  more  biologically  relevant  (see  Dis- 
cussion). The  first  larval  stage  of  Homarus  is  sometimes 
referred  to  as  a  "mysis"  (Shiino,  1988)  based  on  the  num- 
ber of  its  appendages,  or  a  first  "zoea"  because  it  locomotes 
with  its  thoracic  appendages  (Anderson,  1982;  William- 
son, 1982).  Finally,  "embryogenesis,"  "prehatch,"  and 
"egg  development"  are  used  interchangeably,  although 
the  latter  part  of  egg  development  in  Homarus  is  devoted 
to  larval  organogenesis  rather  than  to  embryogenesis 
strictly  defined. 

Results 

/.   Timing  of  development,  sequence  of  events, 
and  characterization  of  embryonic 
and  early  postembryonic  stages 

In  the  following  account  of  the  embryogenesis  of  Hom- 
arus americanus,  the  sequence  of  developmental  events 
and  morphometric  data  on  eye  index  (El)  and  cephalo- 
thoracic  length  (Figs.  1,  2)  were  obtained  by  studying  the 
experimental  brood,  whereas  the  illustration  and  char- 
acterization of  equally  spaced  embryonic  and  early  post- 
embryonic  stages  was  achieved  by  studying  many  clutches 


Eye 

index  (um)               %  developmen 

t 

600 

TIOO 

•A"" 

500 

:                  $  - 



T  I  a  £     so 

400 

gnC.' 

gS   *                     eo 

S1 

,/* 

300 

D 

I1*1   i' 

1° 

''''       /''',, 

9                                                                40  . 

200 

H 

//''/ 

20 

•''''/'  ''''''' 

100 

:  f                 '- 

1;^ 

0 

i     i     i     i     §     i     i     i 

'"• 

Age  (days) 


20   40   60   80   100  120  140  160  180 


Metaiiaupliar  Molt 

Stage  D2 -3  and  hatching 

Haemolymph  blue 

Setae  invaginating  in  telson 

Stage  Di 

(15+15)  setae  on  telson 

Yolk  in  1/2  of  egg  volume 

Tailflips 

Eyes  oval 

Stage  Do 

Red  pigment 

Intestinal  granules 

Heart  beats 

Eye  pigment 

Naupliar  molt 

(6+6)  setae  on  telson 

Twitches 

Fertilization 


5 
o.  c 


0, 


03  <U 
~  tfl 
O,  * 


Figure  I .  Eye  index  versus  age  of  the  experimental  brood  of  Homarus  americanus.  maintained  at  1 8°C. 
Developmental  landmarks  are  indicated  along  a  percent-scale  based  on  the  eye  index.  Perkins'  eye  index 
(1972)  is  the  mean  of  the  length  and  the  width  of  the  screening  pigment  spot  in  the  lateral  eyes.  Each  data 
point  represents  the  mean  of  measurements  on  five  individuals  ot  the  experimental  brood  ±  one  standard 
deviation. 


EMBRYONIC  DEVELOPMENT  OF  THE  LOBSTER 


359 


2.4 
2.2 

-        Q   Egg 
i    Cephalothorax 

2.0 

:                              B    i|I 

N 

co 

1.8 
1.6 
1.4 
1.2 
1.0 

;e^°D°  ftf11 
;      ^  ^*r 

0.8 

i*1 

0.6 

:    I1 

0.4 

~  * 

0.2 
00 

-                                               EI    (|im) 

c 

100         200        300         400         500         600 

( 

)       10     20     30     40      50     60     70     80     90    100 

%  Development 

Figure  2.  Greatest  axis  of  egg  and  cephalothoracic  length  versus  eye 
index  and  percent-development  scale,  in  Homarits  americanus.  Greatest 
axis  of  egg:  each  data  point  represents  the  mean  of  five  measurements 
±  one  standard  deviation  (data  points  from  different  broods).  Cephalo- 
thoracic length:  the  length  of  each  bar  represents  one  standard  deviation 
on  each  side  of  the  mean  of  five  measurements  (all  individuals  from 
experimental  brood). 


of  eggs  at  different  levels  of  resolution  [whole  eggs  (Figs. 
3,  4),  dissected  embryos  (Fig.  5),  antennulae  (Fig.  6),  and 
telsons  (Figs.  7,  8)].  The  stage  of  appearance  of  develop- 
mental events  is  expressed  in  percent-development  of  total 
embryogenesis.  The  percent-staging  scheme  is  explained 
in  "Materials  and  Methods."  Dates  of  observation  of  the 
experimental  brood,  age,  eye  index,  and  percent-staging 
system  are  related  in  Table  I  and  in  Figure  1 .  The  metan- 
aupliar  molt  cycle  is  described  in  the  second  part  of  "Re- 
sults." 

Sequence  of  events  prior  to  10%  development.  The  first 
organized  structure  to  appear  at  the  surface  of  the  green 
yolk  at  5%  development  (E5%,  estimated  day  8  of  the 
experimental  brood)  is  a  typical  crustacean  nauplius  with 
three  pairs  of  appendages:  the  antennulae,  the  antennae, 
and  the  mandibles.  The  mandibles  are  first  visible  as  two 
dots  medial  to  the  endopods  of  the  antennae.  This  is 
equivalent  to  stage  "M"  of  Bumpus  with  the  eye  lobes 
and  the  thoracoabdominal  process  still  undefined.  At  E6% 
(day  10),  the  optic  lobes  appear  as  a  white  cloud  of  cells, 
and  the  thoracoabdominal  process  is  clearly  outlined.  At 
E8%  (day  12,  stage  "N"  of  Bumpus)  the  optic  lobes  are 
also  delineated,  and  the  embryo  is  easily  separated  from 
the  yolk.  The  dorsal  side  of  the  embryo  is  apposed  to  the 
yolk,  and  the  abdomen  grows  folded  on  the  ventral  side 
of  the  thorax.  The  tip  of  the  abdomen  reaches  the  level 
of  the  mandibles  and  is  beginning  to  part  medially  at  E9% 


(day  15,  stage  "O"  of  Bumpus);  the  buds  of  four  pairs  of 
post  mandibular  appendages  line  the  trunk. 

70%  development  (no  eye  pigment  present:  Figs.  3.4, 
5A,  6A,  7 A).  At  E10%,  the  yolk  occupies  approximately 
95%  of  the  volume  of  the  egg  (Fig.  3A)  whose  greatest 
axis  measures  about  1.6  mm.  The  antennulae  are  unira- 
mous  and  end  with  five  setae  whereas  the  antennae  are 
biramous  with  five  setae  at  the  tip  of  the  exopods  and 
three  setae  at  the  tip  of  the  endopods  (Fig.  6A).  The  ex- 
tremity of  the  abdomen  nearly  reaches  the  level  of  the 
labrum;  the  telson  is  beginning  to  part  but  setae  are  not 
yet  present  at  its  tip.  This  stage  is  equivalent  to  stage  "O" 
of  Bumpus  (1891). 

Sequence  of  events  from  10%  to  20%  development.  At 
El  1%  (day  17)  at  least  8  appendages  are  formed  past  the 
mandibles  and  at  about  this  time,  the  first  twitches  in  the 
two  pairs  of  antennae  occur  upon  dissection.  Pigment  is 
then  visible  in  the  median  eye  under  the  compound  mi- 
croscope. At  about  E 1 2%,  when  pigment  is  already  visible 
in  the  median  eye  but  not  yet  in  the  lateral  eyes,  an  em- 
bryonic molt  occurs  (see  II,  below).  At  least  two  envelopes 
surround  the  antennulae  (Fig.  6B).  and  one  envelope  is 
stretched  at  the  tip  of  the  six  setae  (6  +  6)  on  each  side 
of  the  telson  indicating  the  occurrence  of  a  molt  (Figs. 
7B,  8A).  At  about  El 3%  (day  21),  pigment  is  seen  in  the 
lateral  eyes  as  a  small  dark  crescent  lining  the  posterior 
part  of  the  lobes;  the  eye  index  at  that  stage  is  approxi- 
mately 70  jum.  At  El 4%  (day  23),  the  first  heartbeats  are 
seen  in  approximately  10%  of  the  embryos  examined.  By 
El  7%  (EI  98)  heartbeats  are  seen  in  half  of  the  eggs,  and 
small  refringent  granules  are  present  in  the  intestine. 

20%  development  (EI  =  114  fim;  Figs.  3B,  5B,  6C.  7C). 
Muscular  twitches  are  readily  observed  in  the  antennae, 
around  the  mouth,  and  in  the  abdomen,  and  the  heart  is 
beating.  Between  10  and  20%  of  embryonic  development, 
the  nauplius  has  molted  into  a  metanauplius  (see  II,  be- 
low). This  stage  is  equivalent  to  stage  "P"  of  Bumpus 
( 1 89 1 ),  defined  as  the  stage  when  the  tips  of  the  third  pair 
of  maxillipeds  reach  the  point  of  insertion  of  the  anten- 
nulae and  the  telson  reaches  the  level  between  the  mouth 
and  the  median  eye.  The  telson  is  provided  with  6-7  setae 
on  each  of  the  two  lobes  (Fig.  7C). 

Sequence  of  events  from  20%  to  30%  development.  At 
E24%  (EI  139),  red  pigment  spots  (chromatophores)  ap- 
pear on  each  side  of  the  brain.  At  E25%  (EI  145)  the 
telson  reaches  the  anterior  edge  of  the  optic  lobe  (stage 
"Q"  of  Bumpus).  The  full  complement  of  appendages 
present  in  the  metanauplius  is  formed  by  E27%  (EI  157), 
and  by  E28%  (EI  161 )  the  telson  reaches  beyond  the  optic 
lobes. 

30%  development  (EI  =  171  ^m;  Figs.  3C.  5C.  6D, 
ID).  The  cephalothorax  of  the  embryo  is  nearly  1  mm 
long.  Red  chromatophores  are  present  on  each  side  of  the 
brain.  A  cluster  of  presumed  sensory  neurons  has  formed 
in  the  exopod  of  each  antennula  and  their  axons  follow 


360 


S.  M.  HELLUY  AND  B.  S.  BELTZ 


60 


H 


70 


80 


EMBRYONIC  DEVELOPMENT  OF  THE  LOBSTER 


361 


the  anterior  edge  of  these  appendages  in  a  bundle  of  a  few 
micrometers  (Fig.  6D).  Serial  plastic  sections  have  shown 
that  the  bundle  of  axons  projects  to  the  olfactory  lobes 
(in  the  deutocerebrum),  which  measure  about  40  nm  in 
diameter  at  that  stage  (unpub.  results).  The  cluster  of  neu- 
rons is  very  similar  to  the  cluster  of  bipolar  sensory  neu- 
rons that  innervate  each  aesthetasc  (olfactory  sensillum) 
in  the  antennulae  of  spiny  lobsters  (Griinert  and  Ache, 
1988).  At  E30%  also,  the  endopod  of  each  antennula 
tipped  with  a  pointed  seta,  is  visible  under  the  cuticle  (Fig. 
6D).  The  endopods  are  freed  after  hatching  when  the  me- 
tanauplius  molts  into  a  first  larval  stage  (LI).  The  ap- 
pearance of  the  endopod  of  the  antennulae  is  the  first 
visible  sign  of  the  formation  of  the  LI  under  the  cuticle 
of  the  metanauplius.  All  postmandibular  appendages  are 
present  and  the  trunk  is  lined  with  six  pairs  of  prominent 
appendages:  a  pair  of  third  maxillipeds  and  five  pairs  of 
walking  legs.  During  the  metanaupliar  phase,  the  trunk 
appendages,  which  are  uniramous,  cannot  be  separated 
from  each  other.  The  tips  of  the  third  maxillipeds  reach 
a  level  between  the  point  of  insertion  of  the  antennulae 
and  the  anterior  edge  of  the  optic  lobes  whereas  the  telson 
is  at  the  level  of  the  anterior  edge  of  the  optic  lobes  [stage 
"Q"  of  Bumpus  (1891)].  In  the  telson,  the  metanaupliar 
cuticle  begins  to  separate  from  the  side  of  the  setae  but 
the  tip  of  these  setae  is  still  attached  to  the  cuticle  (Fig. 
7D);  this  signals  the  start  of  the  premolt  stage  Dn  of  the 
metanaupliar  molt  cycle  (see  II,  below). 

Sequence  of  events  from  30%  to  40%  development.  Stage 
"R"  of  Bumpus  is  reached  between  E30%  and  E37%  when 
the  tip  of  the  third  maxillipeds  is  at  the  level  of  the  an- 
tennae and  the  telson  grows  beyond  the  optic  lobes.  By 
E37%,  the  eye  pigment  spots  have  become  oval  rather 
than  crescent-shaped,  and  red  pigment  granules  line  the 
sides  of  the  nerve  cord. 

40%  development  (El  =  228  urn;  Figs.  3D.  5D,  6E,  IE). 
A  giant  sensillum  (260  /urn)  is  visible  as  a  long  straight 
rod  inverted  at  the  tip  of  the  exopod  of  each  antennula. 
Setae  are  present  at  the  extremities  of  trunk  appendages. 
Red  chromatophores  are  seen  on  the  sides  of  the  nerve 
cord,  on  the  anterior  edge  of  the  optic  lobes,  and  on  the 
growing  carapace.  The  third  maxillipeds  reach  the  anterior 
edge  of  the  optic  lobes,  and  the  telson  reaches  anteriorly 
to  the  optic  lobes.  This  stage  is  more  advanced  than  stage 
"R."  the  most  advanced  stage  described  by  Bumpus 
(1891). 


Sequence  of  events  from  40%  to  50%  development.  The 
red  chromatophores  have  invaded  the  appendages  and 
the  growing  carapace  by  E43%  and  the  abdomen  by  E49%. 

50%  development  (El  =  285  urn;  Figs.  3E,  5E,  IF).  By 
E50%,  some  embryos  perform  very  clear  tailflips  after  re- 
moval of  egg  envelopes;  also,  the  first  caeca  of  the  paired 
digestive  glands  (hepatopancreas)  are  seen,  with  the 
stereomicroscope,  at  the  anterior  end  of  the  midgut  where 
it  comes  in  contact  with  the  mass  of  yolk.  The  rostrum 
of  the  differentiating  LI  is  folded  ventrally  between  the 
optic  lobes  and  is  visible  upon  dissection.  The  gap  between 
the  cuticle  and  the  six  or  seven  most  distal  and  lateral 
setae  on  each  side  of  the  telson  has  widened,  but  the  tips 
of  these  setae  are  still  in  contact  with  the  cuticle  (Fig.  7F). 
Other  setae  are  growing  more  medially  and  more  proxi- 
mally  beneath  the  cuticle  of  the  telson.  By  now,  the  distal 
ends  of  the  third  maxillipeds,  as  well  as  the  telson,  reach 
anteriorly  to  the  level  of  the  optic  lobes. 

Sequence  of  events  from  50%  until  hatching.  There  are 
no  obvious  changes  in  the  general  external  morphology 
of  the  embryo  from  E50%  until  hatching  (£100%).  How- 
ever, the  embryo  grows  dramatically  and  the  structures 
typical  of  the  LI  are  forming  progressively  beneath  the 
cuticle  of  the  metanauplius. 

60%  development  (El  =  342  urn:  Figs.  3F.  5F,  6F,  7G). 
At  this  stage  the  yolk  occupies  about  half  the  volume  of 
the  egg.  At  least  1 0  setae  are  formed  on  each  side  of  the 
telson  (Fig.  7G). 

70%  development  (El  =  399  n>n:  Figs.  3G,  5G,  7H. 
8B).  The  full  complement  of  setae  ( 14  or  1 5  on  each  side) 
of  the  first  larval  stage  is  present  on  the  telson  under  the 
metanaupliar  cuticle;  the  median  spine  begins  to  differ- 
entiate (Figs.  7H,  8B). 

50%  development  (El  =  456  urn:  Figs.  3H,  5H,  6G, 
71).  In  the  telson,  only  the  most  medial  setae  are  in  contact 
with  the  metanaupliar  cuticle.  These  setae  have  not  yet 
assumed  the  shape  of  spines,  and  the  embryos  are  still  in 
stage  DO  of  the  metanaupliar  molt  cycle  (Fig.  71). 

90%  development  (El  =  513  \im;  Figs.  4A,  51,  6H,  7J. 
8C).  The  egg  is  now  enlarging  rapidly,  and  its  largest  axis 
measures  about  2.0  mm  (Fig.  2).  The  yolk  is  turning  yellow 
(Fig.  4A).  The  telson  manifests  a  number  of  dramatic 
changes  (Figs.  7J,  8C).  The  cuticle  has  lifted  entirely  from 
the  setae  and  also  from  the  epidermis  on  the  lateral  sides 
of  the  telson.  The  two  most  lateral  setae  are  now  pointed 
and  sharp  like  spines,  and  they  begin  to  retract.  About  a 
third  of  each  seta  is  visible  beneath  the  tegument.  The 


Figure  3.  Unfixed,  intact  eggs  of  Homarus  americanus  at  (A)  10,  (B)  20,  (C)  30,  (D)  40.  (E)  50,  (F)  60, 
(G)  70,  and  (H)  80%  embryonic  development.  The  figures  in  the  lower  left  corners  refer  to  the  percentage 
of  development.  In  all  photographs,  the  dorsal  side  is  at  the  top,  and  the  head  and  telson  of  the  embryo  are 
on  the  right.  At  10%  development  (E10%),  the  embryo  is  seen  as  a  small  halo  at  the  bottom  part  of  the  egg. 
The  eye  pigment  is  visible  in  the  lateral  eyes  (le)  by  E20%.  The  red  chromatophores  (ch)  already  present  by 
E40%  are  labeled  at  E60%.  The  intestinal  granules  (ig)  are  particularly  clear  at  E70%.  Scale  bar:  500  /jm. 


362 


S.  M.  HELLUY  AND  B.  S.  BELTZ 


Figure  4.  Perihatching  development  of  Homarus  americanus.  In  all  these  photographs  of  unfixed  spec- 
imens, dorsal  side  is  at  the  top,  and  anterior  is  right.  (A)  90%  embryonic  development.  (B)  Embryo  just 
prior  to  hatching  (100%,  blue  embryo):  note  the  blue  tinge  of  the  hemolymph,  the  blue  stomach  (st)  and 
the  red  chromatophores  (ch)  in  which  the  pigment  is  still  concentrated.  (C)  Hatchling:  the  outer  (oe)  egg 
envelope  has  burst,  and  the  telson  (te)  is  piercing  the  inner  egg  envelope;  the  red  pigment  has  spread  in  the 
star-shaped  chromatophores  (ch).  (D)  The  metanauplius  (prelarva,  prezoea)  is  now  free  of  both  outer  (oe) 
and  inner  (ie)  egg  envelopes.  (E)  Early  first  larval  stage  (first  zoea):  the  exuvia  (ex)  of  the  metanauplius  has 
been  sloughed.  (F)  Mature  first  larval  stage:  rostrum,  abdominal  spines,  and  other  acuminate  structures  are 
now  erect.  Scale  bars:  500  nm. 


EMBRYONIC  DEVELOPMENT  OF  THE  LOBSTER 


363 


epidermis  forms  papillae  around  each  seta,  and  appears 
scalloped.  Retraction  of  setae  and  scalloped  epidermis  are 
characteristic  of  stage  D,  (Sasaki,  1984). 

100%  development  (El  =  570  urn;  Figs.  4B.  7K).  At 
this  stage,  just  prior  to  hatching,  the  egg  (2.2  mm)  is 
brightly  colored  (Fig.  4B).  The  stomach  is  deep  blue  and 
the  hemolymph  pale  blue.  The  yolk,  which  has  been  nearly 
entirely  absorbed,  is  yellow  or  pale  green.  The  two  pairs 
of  yolk  caeca  that  were  filling  the  egg  earlier  are  attached 
to  the  digestive  tube  dorsally  by  this  time,  between  the 
pyloric  stomach  and  the  numerous  tubular  digestive 
glands.  The  bilateral  spines  of  the  telson  are  entirely  re- 
tracted, whereas  the  setae  are  only  partially  so. 

Eclosion  of  the  metanauplius  (fiatcliling)  (Figs.  4C,  61, 
7L,  8D).  The  outer  egg  envelope  has  burst.  The  red  pig- 
ment disperses  in  star-shaped  chromatophores  (Fig.  4C). 
The  giant  sensillum  is  everted  and  projects  from  the  exo- 
pods  of  the  antennulae,  but  is  still  confined  within  the 
cuticle  of  the  metanauplius  (Fig.  61).  The  spines  and  setae 
of  the  telson  begin  to  expand  (Figs.  7L  and  8D).  The  epi- 
dermis becomes  very  distinct  and  forms  a  pronounced 
bulging  around  the  invaginated  setae;  these  are  two  char- 
acteristics of  stage  D:_3  of  Sasaki  (1984). 

Free  metanauplius  (prelarva,  prezoea;  Fig.  4D).  The 
metanauplius  is  freed  of  the  two  external  egg  envelopes; 
it  is  mostly  still,  but  occasionally  performs  strong  tail- 
flips.  These  movements  presumably  facilitate  the  molting 
process.  Within  hours  after  the  egg  membranes  are  shed, 
the  metanauplius  molts  into  a  first  larval  stage. 

Molt  of  the  metanauplius  and  emergence  of  first  larval 
stage  (Figs.  4E  and  F,  6J.  7M).  The  exuvia  peels  away 
from  the  metanauplius.  Slowly  the  rostrum,  the  abdom- 
inal spines,  and  all  the  other  acuminate  structures  become 
erect.  The  telson  opens  like  a  fan  into  a  triangular  structure 
(Fig.  7M).  The  setae  evaginate.  The  endopod  of  each  an- 
tennula  is  released  (Fig.  6J),  as  well  as  the  feathery  exo- 
podites  of  the  six  pairs  of  thoracic  appendages.  Each  an- 
tennula  at  hatching  has  one  giant  sensillum  (550  /*m)  and 
three  setae  at  the  tip  of  the  exopod,  and  one  seta  at  the 
tip  of  the  endopod  ( Fig.  6J)  as  reported  by  Herrick  (1895). 
In  brief,  the  curvaceous  metanauplius  molts  into  an 
angular  larva  ready  to  assume  a  pelagic  life. 

//.  Metanaupliar  embryonic  molt  cycle,  growth  curves 
and  developmental  plateau 

Metanaupliar  embryonic  molt  cycle.  At  about  El 2%, 
an  envelope  is  seen  enshrouding  the  telson  (Figs.  7B  and 
8A),  stretched  at  the  tips  of  the  12  (6  +  6)  bilaterally  paired 
setae  on  the  telson  of  the  nauplius.  This  envelope  is 
thought  to  be  the  exuvia  of  the  naupliar  stage;  it  is  flat 
and  was  formed  in  the  nauplius  when  the  tip  of  the  ab- 
domen had  not  yet  acquired  any  setae.  The  metanauplius 
that  emerges  at  the  naupliar  molt  has  been  forming  during 
the  naupliar  stage.  From  this  molt  until  the  emergence  of 


the  first  larval  stage  after  hatching  (metanaupliar  molt), 
a  complete  molt  cycle  is  observed  in  the  setal  changes  of 
the  telson.  The  cuticle  begins  to  lift  away  from  the  telson 
at  about  E30%  (Fig.  7D)  when  the  metanauplius  enters 
stage  DO  .  The  shape  of  the  6  +  6  setae  on  the  telson  of 
the  metanauplius  is  well  defined  on  this  cuticle.  Setae  form 
then  medially  and  proximally,  and  by  E70%  (Fig.  7H, 
8B),  the  full  complement  of  setae  of  the  first  larval  stage 
(15  +  15)  is  formed.  Between  E80%  (Fig.  71)  and  E90% 
(Fig.  7J),  dramatic  setal  and  tegumentary  changes  occur 
as  the  metanauplius  enters  stage  D, .  Particularly  striking 
is  the  transformation  of  the  most  lateral  setae  into  straight 
and  sharp  spines  (compare  Figs.  71  and  J,  8B  and  C). 
These  spines  are  invaginating  as  well  as  all  the  setae.  In 
addition,  the  epidermis  becomes  scalloped,  and  the  cuticle 
lifts  from  the  sides  of  the  telson.  Just  prior  to  hatching 
(E100%),  retraction  of  spines  and  setae  is  maximal.  When 
the  outer  egg  envelope  ruptures,  the  bulging  of  the  epi- 
dermis around  the  setae  is  pronounced,  and  the  metanau- 
plius has  entered  stage  D2_3.  After  hatching,  the  metanau- 
plius molts  into  the  first  stage  larva  and  the  cuticle  which 
is  discarded  still  has  the  typical  metanaupliar  shape  with 
the  imprint  of  the  (6  +  6)  metanaupliar  setae. 

Growth  cun'es  and  developmental  plateau.  The  greatest 
axis  of  the  egg  increases  gradually  from  1.6  mm  at  E10% 
to  about  1.8  mm  at  E80%  and  more  rapidly  to  2.2  mm 
at  hatching  (Fig.  2).  In  the  experimental  brood  raised  at 
1 8°C,  both  the  eye  index  (Fig.  1 )  and  the  cephalothoracic 
length  showed  a  logarithmic  growth  from  the  first  time 
these  variables  could  be  measured  to  approximately  day 
110.  In  Figure  2,  cephalothoracic  length  appears  linear 
because  it  is  expressed  as  a  function  of  the  eye  index.  The 
eggs  of  the  experimental  brood  reached  a  developmental 
plateau  at  an  eye  index  of  about  474  (E82%).  Until  this 
stage,  development  of  the  eggs  was  synchronous  with  little 
interindividual  variability  within  the  brood.  This  was 
shown  by  the  low  standard  deviation  of  the  eye  index  and 
of  the  cephalothoraxic  length  (Figs.  1,  2).  Until  82%,  all 
eggs  were  "green"  and  taken  at  random.  However,  after 
E82%,  the  population  was  no  longer  homogeneous,  and 
the  naked  eye  could  distinguish  by  size  and  color  three 
categories  of  eggs:  "green,"  "yellowish"  (Fig.  4A),  and 
"blue"  (Fig.  4B).  In  addition,  the  eggs  hatched  over  a  pe- 
riod of  about  a  month,  again  indicating  significant  vari- 
ability between  eggs  in  a  single  brood.  After  E82%  it  was 
no  longer  possible  to  choose  eggs  randomly  for  observa- 
tion; to  assign  an  approximate  age  to  each  of  these  stages, 
"yellowish  eggs"  were  examined  on  day  152  when  the 
majority  of  eggs  had  reached  this  stage,  and  "blue  eggs" 
(the  hatching  stage)  were  examined  a  week  later.  Indi- 
vidual eggs  took  about  two  weeks  to  change  from  "green" 
to  "blue"  eggs  at  18°C. 

Discussion 

In  the  present  paper,  different  aspects  of  the  embryonic 
development  ofHomams  americanus  are  examined  from 


he 


EMBRYONIC  DEVELOPMENT  OF  THE  LOBSTER 


365 


the  formation  of  the  naupliar  stage  until  the  emergence 
of  the  first  larval  stage.  In  the  discussion  that  follows,  the 
percent-staging  scheme  presented  is  compared  to  that  used 
in  other  invertebrate  systems,  and  anatomical  and  mor- 
phological observations  of  earlier  authors  are  related  to 
that  staging  system.  The  developmental  plateau  in  the 
growth  curve  of  the  eggs  is  discussed  in  the  context  of  the 
embryonic  molt  cycle.  The  occurrence  of  embryonic  molt 
cycles  in  other  crustaceans  is  reviewed  and  the  significance 
of  the  prelarva  debated. 

Staging  system 

Embryonic  studies  on  invertebrates  have  used  staging 
systems  based  upon  particular  developmental  events,  and 
arbitrary  notations  such  as  letters  or  figures  (Bumpus, 
1891;FigueiredoandBarraca,  1963;  Fernandez.  1980)  to 
demarcate  stages.  Nevertheless,  when  intermediate  stages 
are  likely  to  be  needed,  a  continuous  rather  than  incre- 
mental staging  system,  and  in  particular  a  percent-staging 
system,  is  more  flexible  and  communicable  (Bentley  et 
a/..  1979).  In  addition,  a  percent-staging  method  takes 
into  account  the  entire  embryonic  life  of  the  organism 
without  ignoring  periods  when  no  particular  biological 
events  seem  noticeable.  For  example,  Bumpus  (1891), 
Herrick  (1895),  and  Templeman  (1940)  observed  Horn- 
arm  eggs  only  until  the  lateral  eye  pigment  spots  became 
oval,  about  40%  embryonic  development  in  the  present 
study. 

Age  in  warm-blooded  animals  is  generally  a  good  in- 
dicator of  the  stage  of  development,  but  in  invertebrates 
the  rate  of  development  is  strongly  dependent  on  tem- 
perature. To  circumvent  this  problem,  some  methods  have 
relied  on  a  percent-staging  scale  of  total  embryonic  time 
(Schistocerca:  Bentley  et  ai.  1979;  Helisoma:  McKenney 
and  Goldberg,  1989;  Cherax:  Sandeman  and  Sandeman, 
in  press).  In  these  studies,  the  staging  scale  is  "calibrated" 
at  a  given  constant  temperature.  The  time  from  fertiliza- 
tion to  hatching  is  then  transformed  into  a  percent-staging 
scale.  This  scale  is  applicable  to  animals  raised  at  other 
temperatures  because  all  developmental  events  are  com- 
pressed or  expanded  proportionally,  depending  upon  the 
temperature. 

However,  as  pointed  out  by  Bentley  et  ai  (1979),  a 
staging  system  based  on  percent  of  total  time  of  embryo- 
genesis  cannot  be  applied  in  species  with  a  period  of  de- 
velopmental arrest.  Homarus  embryos  manifest  a  period 


of  arrested  development  in  natural  conditions  (Perkins, 
1972)  and  at  constant  temperature  (present  study)  and 
these  results  indicate  that  factors  other  than  temperature 
have  a  strong  influence  on  the  rate  of  embryonic  devel- 
opment in  lobsters.  Therefore,  we  have  used  a  morpho- 
metric  index,  the  size  of  the  screening  pigment  spot  in 
the  lateral  eyes  (the  eye  index  of  Perkins,  1972).  as  the 
basis  for  a  percent-staging  scheme.  The  eye  index  has  been 
used  as  an  indicator  of  developmental  stage  in  a  number 
of  embryonic  studies  (Schuur  et  ai,  1976;  Hepper  and 
Gough,  1978;  Cole  and  Lang,  1980;  Sasaki,  1984;  Sasaki 
et  ai,  1986;  Beltz  and  Kravitz,  1987;  Beltz  et  ai,  1990; 
Helluy  and  Beltz,  1990;  Meier  and  Reichert,  1990). 

There  are  two  obvious  limitations  of  a  staging  system 
based  upon  the  eye  index.  First,  the  eye  pigment  does  not 
appear  until  approximately  three  weeks  after  egg  extru- 
sion, and  at  the  first  possible  measurement  is  about  70 
nm.  To  calibrate  the  relatively  brief  period  from  extrusion 
to  appearance  of  eye  pigment,  a  time-staging  analysis  has 
been  used  (see  Materials  and  Methods).  Thus,  a  percent- 
staging  system  has  been  established  that  covers  the  entire 
embryonic  period,  from  extrusion  until  hatching.  The 
second  drawback  of  the  proposed  method  is  related  to  the 
variation  of  the  eye  index  at  hatching.  Perkins  (1972)  re- 
ported that  the  eye  index  at  hatching  is  560  urn.  Just  prior 
to  hatching  in  four  broods  that  we  have  examined,  it 
ranged  from  570  urn  to  586  /^m  and,  taking  into  account 
Perkins'  figure  of  560  /urn,  570  ±  20  ^m  was  chosen  as 
the  eye  index  at  hatching.  This  figure  was  used  as  the  end 
point  in  establishing  the  percent-staging  scale.  The  20  ^m 
variation  represents  a  small  proportion  (3.5%)  of  the  total 
percentage  scale.  The  variation  of  the  eye  index  at  hatching 
could  be  due  to  a  combination  of  factors  such  as  genetic 
variability  or  perturbations  due  to  environmental  con- 
ditions. For  instance,  it  has  been  reported  that  molting 
and  development  of  morphological  characteristics  proceed 
somewhat  independently  in  decapods  (Gore,  1985),  and 
it  is  conceivable  that  the  physiological  changes  that  reg- 
ulate hatching  and  molting  could  be  advanced  or  delayed 
with  respect  to  morphogenesis  and  growth. 

Growth  ciiwes  and  the  developmental  plateau 

Lobster  egg  masses  kept  at  seasonal  water  temperature 
show  a  developmental  plateau  when  the  temperature  is 
low  during  the  winter  months  (Perkins,  1972).  More  sur- 
prising is  the  fact  that  our  experimental  brood,  which  was 


Figure  5.  Ventral  view  of  embryos  of  Homarus  americanus  dissected  from  the  yolk  and  unfixed  at  (A) 
10.  (B)  20,  (C)  30,  (D)  40,  (E)  50,  (F)  60,  (G)  70,  (H)  80,  and  (I)  90%  development.  In  all  photographs,  the 
head  is  at  the  top  and  the  abdomen  is  folded  ventrally  onto  the  thorax.  The  figures  in  the  lower  left  corners 
refer  to  the  percentage  of  development.  The  line  drawing  (J)  represents  a  schematic  metanauplius.  Abbre- 
viations, ab:  abdomen,  anl:  antennula,  an2:  antenna,  ch:  chromatophores,  he:  heart,  ig:  intestinal  granules, 
le:  lateral  eye,  me:  median  eye,  mxp3:  third  maxilliped,  te:  telson.  Scale  bar  in  (F),  valid  from  (A)  to  (I): 
500  Mm. 


366 


S.  M.  HELLUY  AND  B.  S.  BELTZ 

«^^» 


^  an2 

an1 

.' 

-* 


HA 


L1 

Figure  6.  Unfixed  antennulae  of  embryos  of  Hatnants  americamis  at  (A)  10,  (B)  12,  (C)  20,  (D)  30.  (E) 
40,  (F)  60,  (G)  80,  and  (H)  90%  development,  in  (I)  a  hatchling  (HA)  and  in  (J)  a  first  larval  stage  (LI).  In  all 
photographs,  the  figures  in  the  lower  left  corners  refer  to  the  percentage  of  development.  Thick  arrows  point 
at  the  anterior  end  of  the  animals.  The  antenna  (an2)  is  also  shown  in  (A)  and  (C)  in  addition  to  the  antennula 
(an  1 ).  By  30%  development  (E30%),  a  cluster  of  presumed  sensory  neurons  (sn)  and  their  axons  (ax)  are  present 
in  the  antennula  and  are  particularly  visible  at  E40%.  At  E30%,  the  endopod  (edp)  of  the  antennula  and  the 
seta  (se)  at  its  tip  are  growing:  the  organogenesis  of  the  first  larval  stage  has  already  started.  The  endopod 


EMBRYONIC  DEVELOPMENT  OF  THE  LOBSTER 


367 


raised  at  constant  temperature,  also  showed  a  develop- 
mental plateau.  The  arrest  in  development  characterized 
by  a  lack  of  growth  in  either  the  eye  index  or  the  cephalo- 
thoracic  length,  occurred  at  E82%  development  (El  474). 
At  E80%,  the  eggs  are  green  and  the  metanauplii  are  still 
at  stage  D0,  whereas  by  E90%  eggs  are  yellowish  and  have 
entered  stage  D, .  The  heterogeneity  of  the  egg  population 
and  the  changes  in  pace  of  development  observed  in  the 
experimental  brood  prior  to  hatching  were  therefore  re- 
lated to  the  transition  from  D0  to  D,  of  the  metanaupliar 
molt  cycle. 

Developmental  plateaus  also  occur  during  stage  D0  of 
the  molt  cycle  in  juvenile  lobsters  (Aiken,  1973).  The  ar- 
rest in  development  takes  place  at  different  times  during 
stage  D0,  from  the  first  indication  of  epidermal  retraction 
to  maximal  epidermal  retraction.  Lobsters  pause  in  their 
development  during  the  cold  winter  months,  and  the 
transition  to  the  irreversible  stage  D,  does  not  proceed 
until  the  water  warms  up  in  the  spring.  Aiken  (1973)  shows 
that  when  a  lobster  has  passed  beyond  pleopod  stage  2.5 — 
and  therefore  entered  stage  D, — development  then  pro- 
ceeds at  a  rate  regulated  by  temperature.  It  is  possible  that 
the  embryonic  metanauplius  goes  through  the  same  cycle. 
Indeed,  developmental  plateaus  have  been  observed  at 
different  eye  indices  during  stage  D0  (from  350  ^m  to  450 
nm)  in  different  broods  of  eggs  (Thomann,  Beltz,  and 
Helluy,  unpub.  results).  Additionally,  Perkins  (1972)  notes 
that  during  the  winter  months  development  is  arrested  in 
older  eggs  (extruded  early  in  the  summer)  and  still  con- 
tinues in  younger  eggs  extruded  later.  It  appears,  therefore, 
that  in  the  wild  the  eggs  may  spend  a  variable  amount  of 
time  in  stage  D0  (shorter  in  younger  eggs);  in  the  spring, 
internal  and  external  cues  could  trigger  the  transition  from 
DO  to  D, .  This  could  explain  why  extrusion  of  eggs  is  a 
prolonged  event  in  a  population  of  females  in  the  wild, 
whereas  hatching  occurs  during  a  more  limited  period 
(Herrick,  1895;  Perkins,  1972). 

Embryonic  molt  cycles 

In  the  present  study,  evidence  is  presented  for  two  molts 
occurring  in  Homarus  prior  to  the  first  larval  stage  and 
associated  with  the  beginning  and  the  end  of  the  embry- 
onic metanaupliar  stage.  Other  crustaceans  are  also  known 
to  pass  through  molt  cycles  within  the  egg  envelopes 
(Wear,  1974;  Goudeau,  1976;  Goudeau  and  Lachaise, 
1983).  Graf  (1972)  characterizes  the  embryonic  molt  cycle 
of  an  amphipod  on  the  basis  of  changes  in  the  epidermis, 


setae,  and  calcium  storage;  these  changes  parallel  those 
occurring  during  juvenile  and  adult  molt  cycles.  In  Horn- 
ants  both  Herrick  (1895)  and  Bumpus  (1891)  mention 
the  existence  of  embryonic  molts.  Based  on  the  number 
of  membranes  enclosing  the  embryo,  Herrick  (1895,  p. 
183)  presumes  that  at  least  three  embryonic  molts  have 
occurred  by  the  time  the  pigment  appears  in  the  lateral 
eyes  and  predicts  that  many  more  may  take  place  during 
the  long  embryonic  life.  Bumpus  ( 1 89 1 )  notes  that  the 
cuticle  lifts  from  the  embryo  in  the  region  of  the  com- 
pound eye  between  stages  N  and  O,  and  that  a  true  ecdysis 
follows;  this  molt  is  probably  the  same  as  that  observed 
in  the  present  study  around  El 2%,  which  is  slightly  after 
stage  O  of  Bumpus.  Goudeau  et  al.  (1990),  using  electron 
microscopy,  detect  five  envelopes  originating  from  the 
embryo  ofHomams  gam  mams  secreted  beneath  the  inner 
and  outer  egg  envelopes  and  show  that  the  secretion  of 
the  embryonic  envelopes  is  associated  with  high  liters  of 
ecdysteroids;  however,  the  embryos  are  not  staged  and 
the  timing  of  secretion  is  not  studied.  The  fact  that  the 
metanaupliar  cuticle  that  begins  to  lift  from  the  telson  at 
30%  development  possesses  the  imprint  of  the  6  +  6  setae 
that  were  present  at  the  naupliar  molt,  and  the  fact  that 
this  same  metanaupliar  cuticle  with  the  imprint  of  the 
6  +  6  setae  is  discarded  at  the  metanaupliar  molt,  dem- 
onstrate that  there  is  only  one  instar  during  that  period, 
not  the  many  that  were  predicted  by  Herrick  (1895).  The 
progressive  setal  changes  observed  in  the  metanaupliar 
telson  also  support  this  conclusion.  The  several  envelopes 
seen  at  the  level  of  the  antennulae  (Fig.  6B)  and  antennae 
at  stage  El 2%  may  indicate  that  additional  molts  occur 
prior  to  El 2%,  during  the  naupliar  phase. 

Whereas  the  setal  changes  occurring  in  the  telson  of 
the  metanauplius  seem  very  similar  to  those  occurring 
during  the  molt  cycle  of  larval  and  juvenile  lobsters 
(Aiken,  1973;  Rao  et  al.,  1973;  Aiken,  1980;  Sasaki, 
1984),  the  cellular  and  biochemical  changes  in  the  epi- 
dermis and  cuticle  must  be  somewhat  different.  In  the 
growing  metanauplius  there  is  no  fixed  postecdysial 
volume  as  there  is  in  postembryonic  animals.  Indeed, 
the  cephalothorax  of  the  embryo  grows  by  a  factor  of 
about  4  from  the  early  12%  molt  to  the  hatch  molt  (Fig. 
2).  Therefore,  we  presume  that  there  is  no  mineraliza- 
tion of  the  metanaupliar  cuticle.  In  that  respect,  it  has 
also  been  noted  before  (see  review  in  Gore,  1985)  that 
the  prezoeal  cuticle  is  different  from  the  exuvia  of  older 
lobsters. 


of  the  antennula  is  also  seen  under  the  cuticle  of  the  metanauplius  at  E80%,  at  E90%,  and  in  the  hatchling 
but  is  out  of  focus  in  the  photographs  of  other  stages.  The  giant  sensillum  (gs)  at  the  tip  of  the  exopod  of 
the  antennula  is  clearly  seen  forming  inverted  at  E40%  development,  everted  in  the  hatchling  under  the 
cuticle  of  the  metanauplius,  and  free  and  erect  with  three  other  setae  at  the  tip  of  the  exopod  in  the  antennula 
of  the  first  larval  stage.  The  red  pigment  in  the  chromatophores  (ch)  is  seen  concentrated  at  E60%  and  E80% 
and  dispersed  at  E90%.  Scale  bars.  A:  100  jim,  B  and  C:  50  ftm,  D  to  J:  100  Mm. 


368 


S.  M.  HELLUY  AND  B.  S.  BELTZ 


7Q  -i — 

L 


L1 


EMBRYONIC  DEVELOPMENT  OF  THE  LOBSTER 
BCD 


369 


Figure  8.  Line  drawings  of  telsons  of  Hiimuru\  unn'ricaiuix  at  (A)  12,  (B)  70,  (C)  90%  development, 
and  (D)  in  a  hatchling  showing  the  naupliar  ecdysis(Ec)  and  representative  stages  D0.  D, ,  D2_3.  For  photographs 
of  those  stages  and  legend,  see  Figure  7. 


Lobster  embryonic  development  in  perspective 

In  the  present  study,  the  metanaupliar  molt  cycle  and 
the  organogenesis  of  the  first  larval  stage  of  Homarus  are 
examined.  This  aspect  of  development  is  generally  ignored 
in  the  literature.  For  example.  Bumpus  ( 1 89 1 )  studies  the 
early  embryology  of  Homarus  only  until  stage  R  (between 
E30%  and  E40%  development).  Herrick  (1895,  p.  209) 
implies  that  the  organogenesis  of  the  L 1  is  extremely  brief 
and  takes  place  just  prior  to  eclosion  when  he  observes 
that  the  antennulae  "remain  single  until  just  before  the 
time  of  hatching  when  the  inner  branch  of  the  flagellum 
begins  to  grow."  In  the  present  study  the  endopod  (inner 
branch  of  the  flagellum)  of  the  antennulae  is  first  seen  at 
about  30%  development.  Therefore,  our  examination  of 


both  the  antennulae  and  the  telson  indicate  that  the  or- 
ganogenesis of  L 1  begins  early  and  continues  throughout 
the  embryonic  molt  cycle. 

Overlooking  the  embryonic  metanaupliar  molt  cycle 
has  led  to  some  confusion  as  to  the  status  of  the  prezoea 
(prelarval  form).  The  existence  of  a  prezoea  has  been  noted 
in  many  families  of  decapods,  but  its  significance  has  been 
largely  debated  (see  review  in  Gore,  1985).  We  agree  with 
Wear  ( 1974),  who  observes  that  "in  decapods  which  hatch 
at  a  zoea  stage,  the  prezoea]  cuticle  is  associated  with  the 
metanauplius  stage  relegated  to  embryonic  life,  rather  than 
to  the  preceding  nauplius."  This  is  clearly  the  case  in 
Homarus:  the  ephemeral  prelarva  (prezoea)  is  the  mature 
metanauplius  between  the  moment  it  is  freed  of  the  two 
external  egg  envelopes  and  the  time  it  molts  (Fig.  4D). 


Figure  7.  Telsons  of  embryos  of  Homarus  americanus  (unfixed)  at  (A)  10.  (B)  12,  (C)  20,  (D)  30,  (E) 
40,  (F)  50.  (G)  60.  (H)  70,  (I)  80,  (J)  90,  (K)  100%  development,  in  (L)  a  hatchling  (HA),  and  (M)  a  first 
larva]  stage  (LI ).  Panel  M  is  a  montage.  In  all  photographs,  distal  is  at  the  top.  The  figures  in  the  lower  left 
corners  refer  to  the  percentage  of  development.  The  tegumentary  and  setal  changes  typical  of  different  stages 
of  the  molt  cycle  as  described  by  Aiken  (1973  and  1980).  Rao  el  al.  (1973).  and  Sasaki  (1984)  in  larval, 
juvenile,  and  adult  lobsters  are  indicated  with  an  asterisk  in  the  following  text.  At  about  12%  development 
(El  2%).  an  embryonic  exuvia  is  lifting  from  the  telson  of  the  nauplius  (arrows).  The  telson  of  the  metanauplius 
forming  under  that  exuvia  is  provided  with  6  +  6  setae.  At  £30%.  the  metanaupliar  cuticle  begins  to  separate 
from  the  side  of  the  setae*  but  the  tips  of  these  setae  are  still  attached  to  the  cuticle  (arrows);  this  stage  is 
equivalent  to  the  premolt  stage  D0  of  the  molting  cycle  of  older  lobsters.  During  the  metanaupliar  molt 
cycle,  the  setae  present  on  the  triangular  telson  of  the  first  larval  stage  (first  zoea)  are  forming  gradually, 
proximally  and  medially  in  the  telson  of  the  metanauplius.  By  E60%  at  least  10  +  10  setae  (arrows)  are 
visible  under  the  metanaupliar  cuticle.  At  E70%,  the  full  complement  of  setae  of  the  first  larval  stage  ( 1 5 
+  15)  is  formed.  Between  E80%  and  E90%,  dramatic  setal  and  tegumentary  changes  occur.  At  E90%  the 
epidermis  is  scalloped*  (black  line):  setae  and  lateral  spines  are  invaginating*  (arrows)  and  the  cuticle  lifts 
from  the  sides  of  the  telson  (arrowheads);  this  stage  is  equivalent  to  D,.  Just  prior  to  hatching  (E100%) 
retraction  of  spines  and  setae  is  maximum.  Note  the  crumpled  tissue  at  the  base  of  the  lateral  spines  (arrow). 
In  the  hatchling  (HA),  this  tissue  forms  a  dark  ring  (arrow),  the  lateral  spines  are  half  extended,  the  epidermis 
is  very  distinct*  and  the  bulging  of  the  epidermis  around  the  setae  is  pronounced*  (circle)  which  is  characteristic 
of  stage  D2_3.  Ecdysis  takes  place  thereafter,  and  the  metanaupliar  cuticle  bearing  the  shape  of  the  6  +  6 
metanaupliar  setae  (arrows)  is  shed.  After  ecdysis  the  triangular  telson  of  the  first  larval  stage  (LI )  unfolds. 
Note  that  two  individuals  (J  and  L)  had  two  lateral  spines  on  one  side.  Scale  bars,  A  to  D:  50  ion,  E  to  M: 
100  /im. 


370 


S.  M.  HELLUY  AND  B.  S.  BELTZ 


One  interpretation  of  the  coupling  of  hatching  and  molting 
is  that  hatching  is  actually  a  by-product  of  the  molting 
process.  For  instance,  the  extension  of  the  lateral  spines 
of  the  telson  (Fig.  7L)  could  provoke  the  breaking  of  the 
inner  and  outer  egg  envelopes  (Fig.  4C,  D).  Indeed,  the 
lateral  setae  of  the  telson  are  smooth  and  extended  until 
80%  development  (Fig.  71),  begin  to  invaginate  as  soon 
as  they  become  sharp  (Figs.  7J,  8C),  are  entirely  invagi- 
nated  in  the  blue  embryo  (El 00%)  just  prior  to  hatching 
(Fig.  7K),  and  are  seen  half  evaginated  in  the  hatchling 
(Figs.  7L,  8D)  whose  telson  has  just  pierced  the  egg  en- 
velopes (Fig.  4C).  No  other  part  of  the  mature  meta- 
nauplius  is  quite  as  hard  and  sharp  as  the  lateral  spines 
of  the  telson  and  it  is  therefore  possible  that  the  extension 
of  these  spines  triggers  the  rupture  of  the  egg  envelopes. 
By  this  means,  the  metanauplius  at  the  end  of  its  molt 
cycle  would  precipitate  hatching,  and  the  beating  of  the 
pleopods  of  the  mother  would  help  the  larva  to  slip  out 
of  its  swaddling  envelopes. 

Molt  cycles  in  postembryonic  crustaceans  are  under 
hormonal  control,  and  it  is  likely  that  embryonic  molt 
cycles  are  regulated  in  a  similar  way.  The  circulating 
molting  hormone  is  an  ecdysteroid  whose  liters  increase 
in  DO  and  peak  in  D2-Di  in  Homarus  (Snyder  and  Chang, 
1991)  and  in  D,  in  Penaeus  (Chan  et  a/..  1988).  Because 
steroids  influence  neuronal  development  and  survival  in 
other  systems  (Weeks  and  Truman,  1986),  the  awareness 
of  embryonic  molts  and  the  prediction  of  the  timing  of 
potential  changes  in  steroid  levels  could  be  critical  for 
future  developmental  neurobiological  studies. 

The  nauplius  is  a  form  common  to  all  crustaceans,  and 
in  some  taxa  (e.g.,  Cirripedia,  Anostraca)  eggs  hatch  as 
nauplii.  In  other  species,  the  naupliar  stage  is  followed 
within  the  egg  envelopes  by  the  organogenesis  of  a  more 
complex  body  form  characterized  by  the  morphogenesis 
and  growth  of  the  postmandibular  region  (Anderson, 
1979,  1982;  Weygoldt,  1979;  Williamson,  1982;  Gore, 
1985;  Schram,  1986;  Shiino,  1988).  It  has  been  shown  in 
several  taxa  [amphipods:  Graf  (1972);  isopods:  Goudeau 
(1976);  decapods:  Wear  (1974),  and  the  present  study] 
that  envelopes  equated  to  embryonic  exuvia  are  found 
during  embryonic  life  as  the  postmandibular  region  dif- 
ferentiates. In  amphipods  (Graf,  1972)  and  decapods 
(present  study),  embryonic  molt  cycles  are  demonstrated 
with  the  progressive  setal  and  tegumentary  changes  oc- 
curring in  the  telson.  The  existence  of  embryonic  molt 
cycles  in  different  taxa  suggests  that  the  relegation  oflarval 
stages  to  life  in  the  egg,  or,  rather,  the  delay  of  hatching 
with  regard  to  molting,  is  a  widespread  and  distinctive 
evolutionary  strategy  in  crustaceans.  Besides  leading  to 
evolutionary  considerations,  the  characterization  of  the 
metanaupliar  molt  cycle  and  the  percent-staging  scheme 
for  lobster  eggs  should  lend  added  insight  in  future  in- 
vestigations of  neural,  physiological,  and  ecological  aspects 
of  Homarus  embryonic  life. 


Acknowledgments 

We  wish  to  thank  Maureen  Ruchhoeft  for  her  kind  and 
skillful  help,  Joe  Gagliardi  and  Kay  Leland  for  printing 
photographic  plates,  Michael  Syslo  and  Kevin  Johnson 
from  the  Massachusetts  State  Lobster  Hatchery  who  pro- 
vided the  egg-bearing  female  lobsters,  as  well  as  Colleen 
Boggs  and  Tom  Coffee  who  maintained  them  at  the  New 
England  Aquarium.  (Supported  by  NSF-BNS-87 18938, 
NIH-NS  25915,  and  NSF-  Presidential  Young  Investigator 
Award  BNS-  8958169  to  B.S.B.) 

Literature  Cited 

Aiken,  D.  E.  1973.  Proecdysis,  setal  development,  and  molt  prediction 
in  the  American  lobster  (Homarus  americanus).  J.  Fish.  Res.  Board 
Can.  30:  1337-1344. 

Aiken,  D.  E.  1980.  Molting  and  growth.  Pp.  91-163  in  The  Biology 
anil  Management  of  Lobsters.  Vol.  1.  J.  S.  Cobb  and  B.  F.  Phillips, 
eds..  Academic  Press,  New  York. 

Aiken,  D.  E.,  and  S.  L.  \\addy.  1980.  Reproductive  biology.  Pp.  2 1 5- 
276  in  The  Biology  and  Management  of  Lobsters,  Vol.  1.  J.  S.  Cobb 
and  B.  F.  Phillips,  eds..  Academic  Press,  New  York 

Anderson,  D.  T.  1979.  Embryos,  fate  maps,  and  the  phytogeny  of  ar- 
thropods. Pp.  59-105  in  Arthropod  Phytogeny.  A.  P.  Gupta,  ed.,  Van 
Nostrand-Remhold,  Princeton.  NJ. 

Anderson,  D.  T.  1982.  Embryology.  Pp.  1-41  in  The  Biology  of  Crus- 
tacea, vol.  2,  Embryology.  Morphology,  and  Genetics.  L.  G.  Abele. 
ed.  Academic  Press.  New  York. 

Beltz,  B.  S.,  and  E.  A.  Kravitz.  1987.  Physiological  identification,  mor- 
phological analysis,  and  development  of  identified  serotonin-proctolin 
containing  neurons  in  the  lobster  ventral  nerve  cord.  J.  Neurosci.  7: 
533-546. 

Beltz,  B.  S.,  M.  S.  Pontes,  S.  M.  Helluy,  and  E.  A.  Kravitz. 
1990.  Patterns  of  appearance  of  serotonin  and  proctolin  immu- 
noreactivities  in  the  developing  nervous  system  of  the  American  lob- 
ster. J.  Neurohiol.  21:  521-542. 

Bentley,  D.,  H.  Keshishian,  M.  Shankland,  and  A.  Toroian-Raymond. 
1979.  Quantitative  staging  of  embryonic  development  of  the  grass- 
hopper, Schistocerca  nil  ens.  J  Embryol.  E\p.  Morphol.  54:  47-74. 

Bumpus,  H.C.  1891.  The  embryology  of  the  American  lobster.  /  Mor- 
phol. 5:  215-262. 

Burrage,  T.  G.  1978.  Fine  structural  development  and  activity  in  the 
heart  and  midgut  of  the  embryonic  lobster.  Homarus  americanus 
(Milne-Edwards).  Ph.  D.  thesis,  Clark  University,  Worcester.  MA. 

Burrage,  T.  G.,  and  R.  G.  Sherman.  1979.  Formation  of  sarcomeres 
in  the  embryonic  heart  of  the  lobster.  Cell  Tissue  Res  198:  477-486. 

Chan,  S.-M.,  S.  M.  Rankin,  and  L.  L.  Keeley.  1988.  Characterization 
of  the  molt  stages  in  Penaeus  vannamei:  setogenesis  and  hemolymph 
levels  of  total  protein,  ecdysteroids,  and  glucose.  Biol.  Bull.  175:  185- 
192. 

Charmantier,  G.  1987.  Le  developpement  larvaire  et  la  metamorphose 
chez  les  Homards  (Crustacea,  Decapoda).  Oceanis  13:  137-165. 

Charmantier,  G.,  and  D.  E.  Aiken.  1987.  Osmotic  regulation  in  late 
embryos  and  prelarvae  of  the  American  lobster  Homarus  americanus 
H.  Milne-Edwards,  1837  (Crustacea,  Decapoda).  /  Exp.  Mar.  Biol. 
Ecol  109:  101-108. 

Cole,  J.  J.,  and  F.  Lang.  1980.  Spontaneous  and  evoked  postsynaptic 
potentials  in  an  embryonic  neuromuscular  system  of  the  lobster. 
Homunis  americanus.  J.  Neurobiol.  11:459-470. 

Davis,  C.  C.  1964.  A  study  of  the  hatching  process  in  aquatic  inver- 
tebrates. XIII.  Events  of  eclosion  in  the  American  lobster,  Homarus 
americanus  Milne-Edwards  (Astacura,  Homaridae).  Am.  Midi.  Nat. 
72:203-210. 


EMBRYONIC  DEVELOPMENT  OF  THE  LOBSTER 


371 


Drach,  P.  1939.  Mue  et  cycle  d'intermue  chez  les  Crustaces  Decapodes. 
Ann  Insl  Oceanogr.  19:  103-392. 

Drach,  P.,  and  C.  TchernigovtzefT.  1967.  Sur  la  methode  de  determi- 
nation des  stades  d'intermue  et  son  application  generale  aux  crustaces. 
Vie  Milieu  A  18:  595-610. 

Ennis,  G.  P.  1975.  Observations  on  hatching  and  larval  release  in  the 
lobster  Homanis  americanus.  J  Fish.  Res.  Board  Can  32:  2210- 
2213. 

Fernandez,  J.  1980.  Embryonic  development  of  the  glossiphoniid  leech 
Theromyian  rude:  characterization  of  developmental  stages.  Dev.  Bio/ 
76:  245-262. 

Kigueiredo,  M.  J.,  and  I.  F.  Barraca.  1963.  Contnbuicao  para  o  con- 
pecimento  da  pesca  e  da  biologia  do  Lagostim  (Nephrops  norvegicus 
L.)  na  Costa  Portuguesa.  Noias  Eslnd.  Insl.  Bin/.  Maril.  (Lisb.)  28: 
1-28. 

Gore,  R.  H.  1985.  Molting  and  growth  in  decapod  larvae.  Crust.  Issues 
2:  1-65. 

Goudeau.  M.  1976.  Secretion  of  embryonic  envelopes  and  embryonic 
molting  cycles  in  Henuoniscus  baluni  Buchholtz,  Isopodu,  Epicaridea. 
/  Morphol.  148:427-451. 

Goudeau,  M.,  and  F.  Lachaise.  1983.  Structure  of  the  egg  funiculus 
and  deposition  of  embryonic  envelopes  in  a  crab.  Tissue  and  Cell 
15:  47-62. 

Goudeau,  M.,  F.  Lachaise,  G.  Carpcnlier,  and  B.  Goxe.  1990.  High 
liters  of  ecdysteroids  are  associated  with  the  secretory  process  of  em- 
bryonic envelopes  in  the  European  lobster.  Tissue  and  Cell  22:  269- 
281. 

Govind,  C.  K.  1989.  Asymmetry  in  lobster  claws.  Am.  Sci.  77:  468- 
474. 

Graf,  F.  1972.  Stockage  de  calcium  et  formation  des  soies  chez  1'em- 
bryon  d'Orehestia  (Crustace.  Amphipode,  Talitride).  Notion  d'in- 
termue embryonnaire.  C.  R  Acad.  Sc.  Sene  D  275:  1669-1672. 

Griinert.  I'.,  and  B.  \V.  Ache.  1988.  infrastructure  of  the  aesthetasc 
(olfactory)  sensilla  of  the  spiny  lobster.  Panulinis  argus.  Cell  Tissue 
Res.  251:95-103. 

llelluy,  S.,  and  B.  S.  Beltz.  1990.  Stages  in  the  embryonic  development 
of  the  American  lobster  Homanis  americanus  with  special  emphasis 
on  its  nervous  system.  Pp.  530-536  in  Frontiers  in  Crustacean  Neu- 
robiologv.  K.  Wiese,  W.  D.  Krenz.  J.  Tautz.  H.  Reichert  and  B. 
Mulloney.  eds.  Birkhaiiser,  Basel,  Boston. 

Hepper,  B.  T.,  and  C.  J.  Gough.  1978.  Fecundity  and  rate  of  embryonic 
development  of  the  lobster,  Homanis  gammanis  (L),  off  the  coast 
of  North  Wales.  /  Cons.  In!  Explor.  Mer  38:  54-57. 

Herrick,  F.  11.  1895.  The  American  lobster:  a  study  of  its  habits  and 
development.  Bull  U.S.  Fish.  Commission  15:  1-252. 

Hoffman,  R.  1977.  The  modulation  contrast  microscope:  principles 
and  performance.  /  Microsc.  110:  205-222. 

Kravitz,  E.  A.  1988.  Hormonal  control  of  behavior:  amines  and  the 
biasing  of  behavioral  output  in  lobsters.  Science  241:  1775-1781. 

McKenney,  K.,  and  J.  I.  Goldberg.  1989.  Helisoma  embryogenesis: 
morphological,  behavioral  and  neural  development.  Soc.  Neurosci. 
Abslr.  15:  1016. 

Meier,  I.,  and  H.  Reichert  1990.  Neuronal  development  in  the  crus- 
tacean nervous  system  studied  by  neuron-specific  antibody  labelling. 
Pp.  523-529  in  Frontiers  in  Crustacean  Neurobio/ogy.  K.  Wiese, 
W.  D.  Krenz,  J.  Tautz,  H.  Reichert,  and  B.  Mulloney,  eds.  Birkhaiiser, 
Basel,  Boston. 


Pandian,  T.  J.  1970a.  Ecophysiological  studies  on  the  developing  eggs 
and  embryos  of  the  European  lobster  Homanis  gammarus.  Mar. 
Bio/.  5:  154-167. 

Pandian,  T.  J.  1970b.  Yolk  utilization  and  hatching  in  the  Canadian 
lobster  Homanis  americanus.  Mar  Bio/.  7:  249-254. 

Perkins,  H.  C.  1972.  Developmental  rates  at  various  temperatures  of 
embryos  of  the  northern  lobster  (Homanis  americanus  Milne-Ed- 
wards). Fish.  Bull  70:  95-99. 

Phillips,  B.  F.,  and  A.  N.  Sastry.  1980.  Larval  Ecology.  Pp.  1 1-57  in 
The  Biology  and  Management  of  Lobsters,  Vol.  2,  J.  S.  Cobb  and 
B.  F.  Phillips,  eds.  Academic  Press,  New  York. 

Rao,  K.  R.,  S.  W.  Fingerman,  and  M.  Fingerman.  1973.  Effects  of 
exogenous  ecdysones  on  the  molt  cycles  of  fourth  and  fifth  stage 
American  lobsters,  Homanis  americanus.  Comp.  Biochem  Physio/. 
A  44:  1105-1120. 

Sandeman,  R.,  and  D.  C.  Sandeman.  1990.  Development  and  identified 
neural  systems  in  the  crayfish  brain.  Pp.  498-508  in  Frontiers  in 
Crustacean  Newohiology,  K.  Wiese,  W.  D.  Krenz,  J.  Tautz,  H. 
Reichert.  and  B.  Mulloney,  eds.  Birkhaiiser,  Basel,  Boston. 

Sandeman,  R.,  and  D.  C.  Sandeman.  1991.  Stages  in  the  development 
of  the  embryo  of  the  freshwater  crayfish  Cherax  deslrucli>r.  Roux's 
Arch.  Dev  Bio/  (in  press). 

Sasaki,  G.  C.  1984.  Biochemical  changes  associated  with  embryonic 
and  larval  development  in  the  american  lobster  Homanis  americanus 
Milne-Edwards.  Ph.  D.  thesis.  Massachusetts  Institute  of  Technology/ 
Woods  Hole  Oceanographic  Institution,  WHOI-84-8. 

Sasaki,  G.  C.,  J.  M.  Capuzzo,  and  P.  Biesiot.  1986.  Nutritional  and 
bioenergetic  considerations  in  the  development  of  the  American  lob- 
ster Homanis  americanus.  Can.  J  Fish.  Aaual  Sci.  43:  231 1-2319. 

Schram,  F.  R.  1986.     Crustacea.  Oxford  University  Press.  New  York. 

Schuur,  A.,  W.  S.  Fisher,  J.  C.  Van  Olst,  J.  Carlberg,  J.  T.  Hughes, 
R.  A.  Shleser,  and  R.  F.  Ford.  1976.  Hatchery  methods  for  the 
production  of  juvenile  lobsters  (Homanis  americanus).  Insl.  Mar. 
Res.  California  48:  1-20. 

Shiino,  S.  M.  1988.  Crustacea.  Pp.  333-388  in  Invertebrate  Embryol- 
ogy. M.  Kume,  and  K.  Dan,  eds.  Garland  Publishing,  New  York. 

Snyder,  M.  J.,  and  E.  S.  Chang.  1991.  Ecdysteroids  in  relation  to  the 
molt  cycle  of  the  American  lobster,  Homanis  americanus.  I.  He- 
molymph  liters  and  metabolites.  Gen.  Comp.  Endocnnoi  81:  133- 
145. 

Templeman,  \V.  1940.  Embryonic  developmental  rates  and  egg  laying 
of  Canadian  lobsters./.  Fish.  Res.  Board  Can  5:  71-83. 

Waddy,  S.  L.,  and  D.  E.  Aiken.  1984.  Broodstock  management  for 
year-round  production  of  larvae  for  culture  of  the  american  lobster. 
Can.  Tech.  Rep  Fish,  and  Aqua.  Sc.  No  1272. 

Wear,  R.  G.  1974.  Incubation  in  British  decapod  Crustacea,  and  the 
effects  of  temperature  on  the  rate  and  success  of  embryonic  devel- 
opment. J.  Mar  Biol.  Assoc.  t/  A  54:  745-762. 

Weeks,  J.  C.,  and  J.  VV.  Truman.  1986.  Steroid  control  of  neuron  and 
muscle  development  during  the  metamorphosis  of  an  insect.  J  Neu- 
robiol  17:  249-267. 

Weygoldt,  P.  1979.  Significance  of  later  embryonic  stages  and  head 
development  in  arthropod  phylogeny.  P.  107  in  Arthropod  Phytogeny. 
A.  P.  Gupta,  ed.  Van  Nostrand-Reinhold.  Princeton,  NJ. 

Wiese  K.,  W.  D.  Krenz,  J.  Tautz,  H.  Reichert,  and  B.  Mulloney. 
1990.  Frontiers  in  Crustacean  Neurobiology.  Birkhaiiser,  Basel. 
Boston. 

Williamson,  D.  I.  1982.  Larval  morphology  and  diversity.  Pp.  43-1 10 
in  The  Biology  oj  Crustacea.  Vol.  2,  Embryology.  Morphology,  and 
Genetics.  L.  G.  Abele,  ed.  Academic  Press,  New  York. 


Reference:  Biol.  Hull  180:  372-386.  (June.  1991) 


How  Do  Temperature  and  Salinity  Affect  Relative 

Rates  of  Growth,  Morphological  Differentiation,  and 

Time  to  Metamorphic  Competence  in  Larvae 

of  the  Marine  Gastropod  Crepidula  planal 

KERRY  M.  ZIMMERMAN  AND  JAN  A.  PECHENIK1 
Biology  Department.  Tufts  University.  Mcdford.  Massachusetts  02155 


Abstract.  The  influence  of  environmental  conditions  on 
rates  of  larval  growth  has  been  documented  many  times 
for  various  marine  mollusks.  But  the  factors  that  influence 
rates  of  morphological  and  physiological  differentiation, 
particularly  the  rate  at  which  larvae  within  a  population 
become  competent  to  metamorphose,  remain  obscure.  In 
four  experiments,  we  reared  larvae  of  the  gastropod  Cre- 
pidula p/ana  at  29°C.  25°C,  and  20°C  at  30  ppt  salinity, 
and  in  two  other  experiments,  in  salinities  between  4-30 
ppt  at  25 °C.  Rates  of  shell  growth  and  morphological 
differentiation,  and  rates  of  becoming  competent  within 
populations  were  recorded.  Larvae  were  considered  to  be 
competent  to  metamorphose  if  they  could  be  stimulated 
to  metamorphose  by  exposure  to  a  high  concentration  of 
KG  (20  mM  above  ambient).  Larvae  consistently  became 
competent  faster  at  higher  temperatures,  but  in  only  one 
of  four  experiments  did  temperature  also  consistently  in- 
crease the  rates  of  growth  and  morphological  differentia- 
tion. Larvae  took  longer  to  become  competent  when 
reared  at  lower  salinities,  but  the  effects  were  poorly  pre- 
dicted by  the  influence  of  salinity  on  rates  of  growth  and 
morphological  differentiation.  Competent  larvae  could 
also  not  be  recognized  by  shell  length;  many  individuals 
were  competent  at  shell  lengths  of  600-800  ^m,  while 
many  other  individuals  were  still  not  competent  at  sizes 
exceeding  1000  urn.  At  29°C,  many  individuals  became 
competent  at  smaller  sizes  than  those  reared  at  lower  tem- 
peratures. Presence  of  gill  filaments  or  shell  brims  also 
did  not  correlate  with  individual  metamorphic  compe- 


Received  26  February  1990;  accepted  9  January  1991. 
'  Please  address  reprint  requests  to  J.  A.  Pechenik. 


tence.  The  data  suggest  that  growth  rate,  rate  of  morpho- 
logical differentiation,  and  time  required  for  larvae  of  C. 
plana  to  become  competent  can  be  uncoupled  markedly 
by  shifts  in  rearing  conditions. 

Introduction 

Competence  is  a  differentiated  state  in  which  larvae  of 
benthic  marine  invertebrates  first  become  capable  of  me- 
tamorphosing in  response  to  environmental  cues  (Crisp, 
1974;Scheltema,  1974;  Chia,  1978;  Hadfield,  1978;  Miller 
and  Hadfield,  1986;  Coon  et  al..  1990;  Fitt  el  al.  1990). 
Metamorphosis  of  gastropod  larvae  is  most  easily  defined 
by  the  loss  of  the  larval  velum,  an  organ  responsible  for 
larval  feeding,  swimming,  and  gas  exchange.  This  trans- 
formation marks  the  transition  from  a  swimming  plank- 
tonic  stage  to  a  largely  sedentary  benthic  stage.  The  time 
required  for  a  larva  to  become  competent  thus  determines 
the  obligate  planktonic  dispersal  period  (Scheltema,  1978; 
Jackson  and  Strathmann,  1981). 

Larvae  are  often  designated  as  competent  based  on  their 
size,  age,  or  the  presence  of  particular  morphological 
characteristics  (Bayne,  1964;  Bayne,  1965;  Bayne,  1971; 
Hickman  and  Gruffydd,  1 97 1 ;  Switzer-Dunlap  and  Had- 
field. 1977;  Hadfield.  1978;  Pechenik,  1984;  Lima  and 
Pechenik,  1985;  Butman  el  al..  1988).  In  at  least  some 
molluscan  species,  however,  such  criteria  may  be  poor 
indicators  of  an  individual's  competence  to  metamor- 
phose. In  the  bivalves  Mytilus  edulis  and  Crassostrea 
gigas.  for  example,  neither  shell  size,  age,  nor  the  presence 
of  eye  spots  guarantee  that  larvae  will  metamorphose  in 
response  to  apparently  appropriate  cues  (Eyster  and 
Pechenik,  1987;  Coon  et  al..  1990).  Similarly,  size  is  an 


372 


COMPETENCE  TO  METAMORPHOSE 


373 


inadequate  indicator  of  metamorphic  competence  for  the 
gastropod  Crepidulafornicata;  larvae  from  a  single  larval 
culture  became  competent  to  metamorphose  at  shell 
lengths  ranging  between  700  and  1000  ^m  (Peehenik  and 
Heyman.  1987.  in  response  to  elevated  KC1  concentra- 
tions). Neither  did  behavioral  changes  successfully  signal 
the  time  at  which  larvae  of  the  opisthobranch  Phestilla 
sibogae  became  metamorphically  competent  in  the  ex- 
periments of  Miller  and  Hadfield  (1986).  There  is  growing 
reason  to  doubt,  then,  that  the  time  required  for  a  larva 
to  become  metamorphically  competent  is  directly  coupled 
to  the  rate  at  which  the  larva  grows  or  develops  most 
other  conspicuous  traits. 

To  date,  few  workers  have  rigorously  documented 
the  rate  at  which  larvae  in  a  population  become  compe- 
tent to  metamorphose,  or  have  considered  the  influ- 
ence of  environmental  factors  on  that  rate.  In  addition, 
the  correspondence  between  the  rates  of  larval  growth 
and  of  attaining  metamorphic  competence  have  been 
poorly  explored.  Under  what  conditions  do  larvae  become 
competent  more  quickly,  and  to  what  extent  can  this  ac- 
celerated attainment  of  competence  be  predicted  from 
the  influence  of  those  conditions  on  rates  of  growth  or 
morphological  differentiation?  Because  larval  metamor- 
phosis can,  for  a  number  of  species,  be  triggered  by  ele- 
vating KC1  ambient  concentration  (Yool  el  ul..  1986; 
Peehenik  and  Heyman,  1987),  the  rate  and  sizes  at 
which  larvae  of  those  species  become  competent  can 
be  determined  experimentally.  The  larvae  of  Crepidula 
fornicata  can  be  induced  to  metamorphose  by  elevated 
K.C1  concentrations  at  about  the  same  age  and  size 
that  larvae  become  responsive  to  adult-conditioned  sea- 
water  and  surfaces  bearing  microbial  films  (Peehenik, 
1980;  Peehenik  and  Heyman,  1987).  The  latter  probably 
serve  as  metamorphic  cues  in  the  field  (McGee  and 
Target,  1989),  but  the  active  constituents  have  not  been 
isolated. 

In  this  paper,  we  report  the  effects  of  temperature  and 
salinity  on  the  rate  of  larval  growth,  the  rate  of  morpho- 
logical differentiation,  and  the  time  required  for  larvae  of 
the  prosobranch  gastropod  Crepidula  plana  to  become 
metamorphically  competent  (as  indicated  by  their  re- 
sponse to  elevated  potassium  concentration).  In  Crepidula 
plana,  virtually  all  larvae  eventually  metamorphose 
"spontaneously" — no  cue  is  deliberately  provided — in 
glassware  that  is  cleaned  and  acid-rinsed  daily  (Lima  and 
Peehenik,  1985).  Thus,  the  maximum  dispersal  potential 
for  these  larvae  depends  on  how  long  metamorphosis  can 
be  delayed  after  they  first  become  competent.  We  therefore 
also  monitored  the  timing  of  "spontaneous"  metamor- 
phosis in  relationship  to  the  onset  of  metamorphic  com- 
petence. We  have  thus  been  able  to  directly  determine 
the  influence  of  temperature  on  the  length  of  time  that 


metamorphosis  can  be  delayed  under  laboratory  condi- 
tions. 

Materials  and  Methods 

Maintenance  of  adults  and  larvae  <>/  Crepidula  plana 

Adult  Crepidula  plana  were  collected  near  Woods  Hole, 
Massachusetts.  We  maintained  adults  at  room  tempera- 
ture (2 1-25°C)  in  1  /urn  filtered  seawater  (collected  at  Na- 
hant,  Massachusetts),  changing  the  seawater  daily.  We  fed 
adults  the  green  unicellular  alga  Dunaliella  tertiolecta 
(clone  DUN)  daily,  until  larval  release.  After  their  release, 
the  larvae  were  isolated  on  a  1 50  ^m  sieve  and  transferred 
to  0.45  ^m  filtered  seawater  (29-30  ppt  salinity).  In  each 
of  the  six  experiments  conducted,  the  larvae  were  all  re- 
leased on  the  same  day,  but  not  necessarily  from  one  fe- 
male. 

Larvae  were  fed  the  naked  flagellate  Isochrysis  sp. 
(Tahitian  strain,  clone  T-ISO)  daily;  seawater  was  changed 
every  other  day.  At  the  start  of  an  experiment  (2-9  days 
after  hatching),  known  numbers  of  larvae  were  randomly 
assigned  to  either  a  20°C,  25°C,  or  29°C  temperature 
incubator  (Percival  Manufacturing)  stable  to  0.1  °C.  Lar- 
vae of  C.  plana  grow  very  slowly  at  temperatures  below 
20°C,  and  29°C  seems  to  be  near  the  upper  lethal  tem- 
perature limit  for  this  species  (Lima  and  Peehenik,  1985). 
All  larvae  were  cultured  on  a  1 1 L:  1 3D  light  cycle.  Larval 
concentrations  were  maintained  below  one  larva  •  ml  '  in 
all  experiments  (I- VI);  the  aim  was  to  maximize  growth 
rates  and  minimize  competition  for  food.  Larvae  were  fed 
1.8  X  105  cells-  ml  '  of  T-ISO  every  other  day  in  Exper- 
iments I  and  II,  and  daily  in  all  subsequent  experiments. 
A  hemacytometer  was  used  to  determine  algal  cell  con- 
centrations. To  monitor  survival,  we  removed  dead  or 
moribund  larvae  from  the  cultures  at  each  water  change. 
Glassware  was  cleaned  with  Bon  Ami  and  rinsed  with 
deionized  water  at  each  water  change. 

Determining  the  influence  of  temperature  on  rates  of 
growth  and  morphological  differentiation 

In  four  experiments,  we  examined  how  temperature 
affects  the  relationship  between  rates  of  larval  growth,  rates 
of  morphological  differentiation,  and  rates  of  becoming 
competent  to  metamorphose.  In  Experiments  I  and  II, 
1 100-1600  larvae  were  reared  at  each  tested  temperature 
(20°C,  25°C,  and  29°C)  in  batch  culture.  Thirty  actively 
swimming  larvae  were  collected  daily  (25°C  and  29°C), 
or  every  other  day  (20°C)  from  the  batch  cultures.  Sea- 
water  volumes  were  adjusted  after  larval  collection  to 
maintain  larval  densities.  In  Experiments  III  and  IV,  we 
determined  the  growth  rates  of  larvae  reared  in  individual 
glass  bowls,  at  densities  also  below  1  larva-  ml" '. 

Larval  shell  lengths  were  measured  at  SOX  using  a  dis- 
secting microscope  equipped  with  an  ocular  micrometer; 


374 


K.  M.  ZIMMERMAN  AND  J.  A.  PECHENIK. 
Table  I 


Inlltieiue  nl  temperature  and  salinilv  on  rale\  ot  larval  shell  i>nmth.  morphological  differentiation  and  heeoming  competent 
lor  larvae  o/  Crepidula  plana 


Experiment 
number 

Temperature 

(°C) 

Salinity 
(ppt) 

Growth  rate 
l/im-day"1) 

Days  to  50%  of          Days  to  50%  of          Days  to  50%  of 
the  population            the  population            the  population 
competent                       gilled                        brimmed 

Mortality 
(n) 

I 

29 

30 

28.4 
(r  =  0.96) 

12.2 

4.0(1620) 

I 

25 

30 

40.0 
(r  =  0.96) 

19.0 

7.0(1120) 

1 

20 

30 

33.8 
(r  =  0.96) 

23.4 

13.0(1620) 

11 

29 

30 

27.9 
(r  =  0.88) 

12.3 

2.5(1300) 

II 

25 

30 

22.5 
(r  =  0.93) 

17.0 

8.0(1300) 

II 

20 

30 

19.2 
(r  =  0.91) 

19.0 

15.0(1300) 

III 

29 

30 

59.1 
(r  =  0.99) 

9.7                             9.7                             10.4 

1.0(1000) 

111 

25 

30 

52.0 
(r2  =  0.89) 

13.4                            10.3                             13.7 

2.0(1000) 

IV 

29 

30 

40.5 
(r  =  0.79) 

11.0                             9.6                             11.3 

1.0(720) 

IV 

25 

30 

43.0 
(r  =  0.91) 

12.6                          11.6                           12.0 

1.0(720) 

IV 

20 

30 

29.4 
(r  =  0.89) 

18.6                          15.4                           18.2 

4.0  (720) 

V 

25 

29 

39.1 
(r  =  0.96) 

—                              —                              — 

18.0(48) 

V 

25 

25 

27.1 
(r  =  0.97) 

_                              _                              _ 

16.0(49) 

V 

25 

19 

15.4 
(r  =  0.91) 

—                              —                              — 

17.5(52) 

VI 

25 

30 

43.6 
(r  =  0.96) 

14.3                            12.5                             14.4 

2.0  (620) 

VI 

25 

25 

35.1 
(r  =  0.94) 

17.6                           13.3                             14.6 

2.0  (620) 

VI 

25 

20 

38.9 
(r2  =  0.94) 

>22.0                            14.0                             15.2 

1.0(620) 

In  Experiments  I  and  II.  larvae  were  fed  every  other  day,  in  all  other  experiments  larvae  were  fed  every  day.  Dashes  indicate  sampling  trom  batch 
culture  (Expts.  I  and  II)  or  data  not  available  (Expt.  V). 


the  maximum  shell  length  was  measured  with  the  larva 
lying  on  its  left  side.  The  presence  of  gill  filaments  and 
the  lateral  shell  brims  characterizing  advanced  larvae  of 
this  species  (Pechenik  and  Lima,  1984)  were  also  noted. 
Growth  rates  (^m  shell  growth -day  ')  were  determined 
by  linear  regression  analysis  of  changes  in  shell  length 


through  time  (SPSS  Inc.,  1988).  The  percentage  of  the 
larval  population  that  was  gilled  or  brimmed  was  plotted 
against  time.  From  these  plots  we  estimated  the  number 
of  days  necessary  after  larvae  were  released  from  their  egg 
masses  for  50%  of  the  larvae  in  a  population  to  become 
gilled  or  brimmed. 


COMPETENCE  TO  METAMORPHOSE 


375 


Table  II 

Influence  <>j  salinity  on  lan-al  sun'ival  and  rule  ol  becoming  competent 
in  Experiment  I ' 


%'  population  competent 

after  7  days  in  each 

Salinity 

salinity  treatment 

(PPT) 

%.  Mortality 

X  ±SD(n) 

29 

18% 

37%     ±4.0(3) 

25 

16% 

10.3%  ±  9.3  (3) 

19 

17.5% 

3.  3%  ±2.9  (3) 

14.5 

31% 

20%     ±  7.2  (3) 

8 

85% 

0%     ±  0.0  (3) 

4 

100%  in  3  days 

— 

At  the  time  of  K.C1  exposure,  larvae  were  16  days  old.  Larvae  were 
introduced  to  the  reduced  salinities  after  9  days  of  culture  at  full-strength 
salinity  (29  ppt). 


Larvae  collected  from  batch  culture  were  preserved  in 
10%  formalin  buffered  with  sodium  borate  (BORAX)  (pH 
~  8.0),  for  later  determination  of  larval  organic  weight; 
larvae  reared  at  different  temperatures  were  stored  sepa- 
rately. Larval  organic  weights  were  determined  as  follows: 
one  or  more  larvae  of  known  shell  lengths  were  placed 
into  pre-weighed  aluminum  pans;  the  preserved  animals 
were  first  rinsed  three  times  with  distilled  water  to  remove 
preservatives  and  salts.  The  animals  were  dried  overnight 
at  60°C  in  a  drying  oven,  then  weighed  to  determine  initial 
total  (inorganic  and  organic)  dry  weights.  The  animals 


100 

o 

- 

J,    4    4 

80 

l 

70 

- 

• 

- 

« 

50 

• 

40 

c 

3 

30 

10 

-f 

n 

I 

i          i          .          i          i          i         //         i 

EXPOSURE   TIME    (HOURS) 


Figure  1.  Influence  of  time  exposed  to  elevated  K.CI  concentration 
on  metamorphosis  of  competent  Crepidula  plana  larvae.  KC1  concen- 
trations were  elevated  by  20  mAJ  at  22-24°C.  Larvae  were  examined 
for  loss  of  velar  lobes  hourly  for  8  h,  then  at  10  h  and  24  h.  Each  point 
represents  the  mean  of  five  replicates,  with  20-21  larvae  per  replicate. 
Vertical  bars  represent  one  standard  deviation.  Average  larval  shell  length 
(±SD)  was  777  Mm  ±  97.8  (n  =  100). 


were  weighed  to  the  nearest  microgram  (^g)  with  a  Cahn 
microbalance  with  desiccant  present  in  the  weighing 
chamber  to  prevent  rehydration.  The  pans  were  reweighed 
after  sample  combustion  in  a  muffle  furnace  at  550°C  for 
6  h;  combustion  did  not  change  the  weight  of  the  alu- 
minum pans.  The  weight  lost  in  combustion  is  equivalent 
to  the  larval  organic  weight.  Individual  body  weights  were 


Table  III 

Results  of  ANACO\'As  for  shell  si:e.  %  of  the  population  competent  10  metamorphose,  %  fully  gilk'd.  or  fully  brimmed 
for  each  experiment  by  temperature  and  salinity  with  age  as  a  covanale 


Experiment  I: 


Experiment  II: 


Experiment  III: 


Experiment  IV: 


Experiment  VI: 


Influence  of  temperature  (at  30  ppt)  on  growth  rate 
Rate  at  which  the  population  became  competent 
Rate  at  which  the  population  became  gilled 
Rate  at  which  the  population  became  brimmed 

Influence  of  temperature  (at  30  ppt)  on  growth  rate 
Rate  at  which  the  population  became  competent 
Rate  at  which  the  population  became  gilled 
Rate  at  which  the  population  became  brimmed 

Influence  of  temperature  (at  30  ppt)  on  growth  rate 
Rate  at  which  the  population  became  competent 
Rate  at  which  the  population  became  gilled 
Rate  at  which  the  population  became  brimmed 

Influence  ot  temperature  (at  30  ppt)  on  growth  rate 
Rate  at  which  the  population  became  competent 
Rate  at  which  the  population  became  gilled 
Rate  at  which  the  population  became  brimmed 

Influence  of  salinity  (at  25°C)  on  growth  rate 
Rate  at  which  the  population  became  competent 
Rate  at  which  the  population  became  gilled 
Rate  at  which  the  population  became  brimmed 


25°C  >  20°C  >  29°C 
29°C>  25°C>  20°C 
29°C  =  25°C  =  20°C 
29°C  =  25°C  =  20°C 

29°C  >  (25°C  =  20°C) 
29°C  >  25°C  >  20°C 
29°C  =  25°C  =  20°C 
29°C  =  25°C  =  20°C 

29°C  >  25°C 
29°C>  25°C 
29°C  >  25°C 
29°C  >  25°C 

25°C  >  29°C  >  20°C 
29°C  >  25°C  >  20°C 
29°C  >  25°C  >  20°C 
29°C  >  25°C  >  20°C 

30  ppt  >  20  ppt  >  25  ppt 
30  ppt  >  25  ppt  >  20  ppt 
30  ppt  >  (25  ppt  =  20  ppt) 
30  ppt  >  25  ppt  >  20  ppt 


All  differences  are  significant  at  P  <  0.05,  and  most  were  significant  at  P  <  0.001. 


376 


K.  M.  ZIMMERMAN  AND  J.  A.  PECHENIK. 


10 


12 


14  IB  18  20 

LARVAL  AGE    (DAYS    FROM  RELEASE) 


24 


Figure  2.  Influence  of  rearing  temperature  on  the  rate  at  which  larvae  became  competent  to  metamorphose 
in  Experiment  I.  Each  point  represents  the  mean  percentage  metamorphosing  in  three  bowls,  with  34-40 
larvae  per  bowl.  Vertical  bars  represent  one  SD  about  the  mean.  Different  letters  represent  larval  populations 
with  different  mean  growth  rates  (A  <  B). 


determined  for  larvae  longer  than  700  ^m;  larvae  less  than 
700  nm  were  pooled  for  weight  determinations. 

Determining  the  effect  of  temperature  on  the  rate  of 
becoming  competent  to  metamorphose 

Pechenik  and  Heyman  (1987)  found  that  elevating  the 
KC1  levels  in  natural  seawater  by  20  mA/  induced  com- 
petent larvae  of  C.  fornicata  to  metamorphose  within  7 
h.  To  determine  whether  the  larvae  of  C.  plana  would 
respond  similarly,  we  exposed  advanced  larvae  of  this 
species  (22-day-old,  770  ±  98  fim  shell  length,  n  =  100) 
to  a  20  mA/  increase  in  KC1  concentration.  We  checked 
hourly  for  larval  metamorphosis  for  the  first  8  h,  then  at 
10  h  and  24  h;  newly  metamorphosed  larvae  were  re- 
moved at  each  observation.  The  experiment  was  con- 
ducted at  22°C,  with  5  replicates  (2 1  larvae  per  replicate). 

To  determine  the  effect  of  temperature  on  the  rate  at 
which  larvae  in  a  given  population  became  competent  to 
metamorphose,  we  monitored  larvae  from  a  temperature 
treatment  until  some  individuals  reached  shell  lengths  of 
about  600  ^m.  At  1-3  day  intervals,  we  then  transferred 
all  larvae  from  three  randomly  chosen  bowls  into  3  bowls 
of  seawater  with  elevated  KC1  concentrations;  30  to  45 
glass  bowls  of  larvae  (20-40  larvae  per  bowl,  depending 
on  the  experiment)  were  used  for  each  temperature  treat- 
ment during  the  course  of  an  experiment.  After  exposing 
larvae  to  the  elevated  KC1  for  6  h,  we  determined  the 
number  of  individuals  that  had  metamorphosed  in  each 
bowl,  and  measured  the  shell  lengths  of  those  that  had 
metamorphosed  and  of  those  that  had  not.  We  also  de- 
termined whether  individuals  had  gills  or  shell  brims.  We 
conducted  Mests  to  determine  whether  there  were  differ- 


ences in  the  mean  shell  lengths  of  competent  and  pre- 
competent  larvae  in  each  temperature  treatment.  The  rate 
at  which  larvae  in  each  population  became  competent 
was  determined  by  linear  regression  analysis.  Significant 
regression  coefficients  (r)  were  obtained  in  all  experiments. 
For  regressions  with  correlation  coefficients  (r)  greater 
than  0.80,  the  number  of  days  for  50%  of  the  larval  pop- 
ulation to  become  competent  was  determined  from  the 
regression.  For  data  with  r2  values  less  than  0.80,  the 
number  of  days  for  the  populations  to  become  50%  com- 
petent was  estimated  by  eye. 

Determining  the  influence  of  temperature  on  maximum 
length  oflamil  life 

Larvae  of  C.  plana  eventually  undergo  "spontaneous" 
metamorphosis  in  the  laboratory,  even  when  maintained 
in  frequently  cleaned  glassware  (Lima  and  Pechenik, 
1985).  Three  bowls  (20-40  larvae  per  bowl,  depending 
on  experiment)  at  each  temperature  were  washed  and  acid- 
rinsed  daily,  at  each  change  of  algal  suspension.  Larvae 
were  examined  daily;  we  counted,  removed,  and  measured 
newly  metamorphosed  snails.  These  data  were  compared 
with  observations  on  the  mean  age  and  size,  at  meta- 
morphosis, of  individuals  cultured  in  bowls  cleaned  only 
every  48  h  ("filmed  bowls").  The  aim  was  to  determine 
whether  biological  films  building  up  over  the  48-h  period 
would  induce  a  greater  number  of  larvae  to  metamor- 
phose. Such  biological  surface  films  have  been  implicated 
as  metamorphic  inducers  in  many  marine  invertebrates 
(Meadows  and  Campbell,  1972;  Scheltema,  1974;  Kirch- 
man  et  al..  1982;  Lima,  1983;  Coon  el  a/..  1985;  Weiner 
etai,  1989). 


COMPETENCE  TO  METAMORPHOSE 


377 


20°C 


Figure  3.     Influence  of  rearing  temperature  on  the  number  of  days  for  50%  of  the  larvae  in  each  treatment 
population  to  become  competent  to  metamorphose  in  Experiments  I-IV. 


The  time  required  for  50%  of  the  population  to  me- 
tamorphose in  the  bowls  cleaned  daily,  minus  the  time 
required  for  50%  of  the  larvae  to  become  competent  in 
parallel  experiments,  was  used  as  an  index  of  capacity  for 
delaying  metamorphosis.  This  cannot  be  used  to  predict 
dispersal  potential  in  the  field,  but  should  enable  us  to 
assess  the  influence  of  temperature  and  salinity  on  the 
physiological  capacity  for  prolonging  larval  life,  and  will 
permit  future  interspecific  comparisons  of  the  physiolog- 
ical capacity  for  delaying  metamorphosis. 

Determining  the  effects  of  salinity  on  rates  of  growth, 
morphological  differentiation,  and  rates  of  becoming 
competent 

In  two  experiments,  we  examined  how  salinity  affected 
the  relationship  between  rates  of  growth,  morphological 
differentiation,  and  becoming  competent.  In  the  first  ex- 
periment, six  salinities  [29,  25,  19,  14.5.  8,  and  4  parts 
per  thousand  (ppt)]  were  used  to  determine  the  salinity 
tolerance  of  larval  C.  plana;  these  salinities  are  equivalent 
to  osmotic  concentrations  of  821,  708,  557,403,  223,  and 
1 16  mOsm,  respectively.  The  five  lowest  salinities  were 
made  by  mixing  0.45  jum  filtered  seawater  with  deionized 
water;  the  29  ppt  seawater  was  composed  solely  of  un- 
diluted 0.45  urn  filtered  seawater.  Osmotic  concentrations 
were  measured  with  a  freezing  point  depression  osmom- 
eter  (Advanced  Instruments,  Inc.).  This  experiment  was 
conducted  at  25°C,  with  three  replicate  bowls  of  20  larvae 
per  bowl  in  each  salinity  treatment.  Water  and  food  were 
replaced  daily.  All  larvae  were  reared  in  full-strength  sea- 
water  for  9  days,  and  then  acclimated  to  lower  salinities 
in  stages  during  1  h.  Shell-less,  moribund,  or  dead  larvae 
were  counted  and  removed  daily.  Shell  lengths  were  mea- 
sured non-destructively  (Pechenik,  1984)  each  day  for 


growth  rate  determinations.  All  larvae  were  exposed  to 
an  increase  of  20  mAl  KC1  on  the  seventh  day  of  the 
experiment  (the  16th  day  of  larval  life)  to  determine  the 
percentage  of  larvae  competent  to  metamorphose  in  each 
salinity. 

Based  on  the  results  of  the  first  experiment,  a  second 
experiment  (Experiment  VI)  was  conducted  at  30,  25,  20 
ppt  (again  at  25°C)  to  examine  more  fully  the  effect  of 
salinity  on  rates  of  growth  and  differentiation.  We  reared 
25  larvae  per  bowl  with  3 1  bowls  per  treatment.  To  min- 
imize the  effects  of  food  supply  on  salinity — algae  are  cul- 
tured at  about  30  ppt — the  algae  were  concentrated  by 
centrifugation  at  3000  X  g  for  12  min  and  then  resus- 
pended  in  seawater  of  the  appropriate  test  salinity  (Pech- 
enik and  Fisher,  1979).  Algal  cells  remained  alive  and 
motile  in  all  salinities.  Every  day,  larval  shell  lengths  were 
measured  non-destructively  from  randomly  selected  bowls 
at  each  salinity,  presence  or  absence  of  gill  filaments  and 
shell  brims  were  simultaneously  noted. 

Periodically,  three  bowls  of  larvae  from  each  salinity 
treatment  were  randomly  selected  and  all  individuals  (20- 
30  larvae  per  bowl)  were  exposed  to  elevated  KC1  con- 
centrations in  seawater  to  assess  metamorphic  compe- 
tence. Larvae  reared  at  30  or  25  ppt  were  exposed  to  an 
increase  of  20  mAf  K.C1  while  those  reared  in  20  ppt  sea- 
water  were  exposed  to  either  a  20  or  a  23  mM  KC1  in- 
crease, to  compensate  for  the  lower  baseline  KC1  concen- 
tration at  the  reduced  salinity.  All  individuals  exposed  to 
KC1  were  measured,  whether  or  not  they  metamorphosed, 
and  were  examined  for  the  presence  of  gill  filaments  and 
shell  brims. 

Statistical  analyses 

Analyses  of  covariance  ( ANACOVA)  were  conducted 
for  each  experiment.  Either  temperature  or  salinity  were 


378 


K.  M.  ZIMMERMAN  AND  J.  A.  PECHENIK 


20°C 

PRE  - 
COMPETENT 


COMPETENT 
25°C 


PRE  - 
COMPETENT 


COMPETENT 


29UC 


PRE  - 

COMPETENT 


400 


n  n       n       am  a  nco(zinii          a 


4  A   •*••*••***•  AA 


1)3 


ana  nan  13  nnrjonca 


D      p   on    a  cnihn        an 


a    n       rzn 


600 


800  1000 

SHELL  LENGTH  (  UM  ) 


1200 


1400 


Figure  4.  A  comparison  of  the  shell  lengths  of  competent  (D)  and  pre-competent  (A)  larvae  ofCrcpidula 
plana  from  Experiment  IV.  The  points  within  each  treatment  represent  the  response  of  larvae  from  three 
bowls  (~60  larvae  per  bowl).  Data  were  taken  when  larvae  at  29°C  were  1 1  days  old  (x  =  50.0%  larvae 
competent;  SD  =  4.3);  larvae  at  25°C  were  13  days  old  (x  =  54.6%  competent;  SD  =  6.4);  larvae  at  20°C 
were  19  days  old  (x  =  51.3%  competent;  SD  =  14.1). 


used  as  independent  variables;  age  (days  from  hatch)  was 
the  covariate;  and  one  of  the  following  was  taken  as  the 
dependent  variable:  percent  of  the  larval  population  com- 
petent to  metamorphose,  percent  of  the  larval  population 
gilled,  percent  of  the  larval  population  with  a  complete 
shell  brim,  or  shell  length  (Table  I)  (Kleinbaum  et  a/., 
1988;  SPSS,  Inc.  1988).  In  Experiments  I  and  II,  the  gill, 
shell  brim,  and  shell  length  data  were  obtained  from  larvae 
in  batch  culture,  whereas  the  rate  at  which  larvae  became 
competent  to  metamorphose  was  determined  with  larvae 
reared  in  glass  bowls.  In  Experiments  III-VI,  all  data  were 
obtained  from  the  larvae  reared  in  glass  bowls.  Percentage 
data  were  arcsine  transformed  prior  to  subsequent  anal- 
ysis, using  the  formula  for  proportions  with  unequal  sam- 
ple sizes  (Draper  and  Smith,  198 1 ). 

Results 

Effects  oj  temperature  and  salinity  on  survival 

Larval  survivorship  was  high  at  all  temperatures  in 
Experiments  I-IV,  with  the  best  survival,  greater  than 
96%,  occurring  at  the  highest  temperature  tested  (29°C) 
(Table  I). 

However,  larvae  were  intolerant  of  very  low  salinities 
(Table  II).  Within  the  first  two  hours  at  4  and  8  ppt,  larvae 
were  found  clumped  together  with  mucus,  mainly  on  the 


bottoms  of  the  rearing  bowls,  with  their  velar  lobes  ex- 
tended and  velar  cilia  moving;  all  treatment  bowls  at 
higher  salinities  (14. 5,  19,  25,  29  ppt)  contained  swimming 
larvae.  On  the  second  day,  at  4  and  8  ppt,  velar  lobes 
appeared  smaller  and  velar  cilia  were  less  visible.  By  the 
third  day,  all  larvae  in  the  4  ppt  seawater  had  died  and 
only  two  larvae  out  of  the  initial  65  survived  at  8  ppt. 
Larval  survivorship  was  good  at  salinities  of  19  ppt  and 
above,  particularly  in  the  second  salinity  experiment  (Ta- 
ble I,  Experiment  VI). 

Effects  of  temperature  on  rates  of  growth  ana" 
morphological  differentiation 

Temperature  had  no  significant  effect  on  size-specific 
organic  weight  at  20  and  25°C  and  at  20  and  29°C  (/- 
tests  between  slopes.  P  >  0.10,  t  =  0.69,  d.f.  =  30  and  / 
=  0.26,  d.f.  =  42,  respectively).  Thus,  a  given  change  in 
shell  length  reflected  comparable  growth  (in  organic 
weight)  for  larvae  at  20  and  25°C,  and  at  20  and  29°C. 
However,  a  given  change  in  shell  length  reflected  greater 
growth  (in  organic  weight)  for  larvae  at  25°C  as  compared 
to  larvae  reared  at  29°C  (/-tests,  P  <  0.05,  t  =  2.05, 
d.f.  =  50). 

The  effect  of  temperature  on  larval  growth  rate  varied 
markedly  among  experiments  (Experiments  I-IV,  Tables 
I  and  III).  There  were  differences  both  in  the  average 


COMPETENCE  TO  METAMORPHOSE 


379 


Table  IV 

Influence  of  temperature  on  age  and  size  at  spontaneous 
metamorphosis  in  glassware  cleaned  daily  (clean  bowls)  and  the  delay 
period  liuinilvr  o!  days  between  when  >0c"f  of  the  population  was 
competent  and  the  mean  age  at  metamorphosis  in  clean  howls) 

Mean  age  (days)  at 

spontaneous  Delay 

Experiment     Temperature     metamorphosis  (clean)  X  ±  SD  period 

number  (°C)  (n)  (days) 


I         29 

19.32  ±  4.9 
(75)  A 

7.16 

1        25 

24.66  ±  3.8 
(98)  B 

5.67 

1        20 

28.66  ±  3.8 
(100)C 

5.26 

11        29 

18.09  ±  5.4 
(99)  A 

5.79 

11        25 

26.86  ±  4.9 
(44)  B 

9.86 

11        20 

30.96  ±3.2 
(73)  C 

12.00 

111        29 

14.49  ±  2.2 
(306)  A 

4.79 

III        25 

16.81  ±  1.6 
(214)  B 

3.40 

IV        29 

16.79  ±  1.7 
(24)  A 

5.79 

IV        25 

18.54  ±  1.4 
(24)  B 

5.90 

IV         20 

24.85  ±  2.6 
(39)  C 

6.21 

Within  each  column,  letters  following  sample  sizes  signify  significantly 
(P  <  0.05)  different  means  within  experiments. 


amount  of  daily  growth  at  a  temperature  and  in  how  tem- 
perature affected  relative  growth  rates.  For  example,  larvae 
grew  the  slowest  (28  ^m-day"1)  at  29°C  in  Experiment 
I,  but  grew  the  fastest  at  29°C  (28  ^m  •  day"1)  in  Exper- 
iment II  (Table  I).  Over  all  experiments,  average  growth 
rates  ranged  between  19  ^m  •  day  '  (Experiment  II,  20°C) 
and  59  Mm-day  '  (Experiment  III,  29°C). 

Larvae  generally  developed  gill  filaments  and  shell 
brims  more  rapidly  at  higher  rearing  temperatures,  al- 
though rates  of  gill  and  brim  formation  were  independent 
of  temperature  in  the  first  two  experiments  (Table  III). 
Note  that  in  Experiment  IV  the  effects  of  temperature  on 
growth  rates  did  not  parallel  those  on  morphological  dif- 
ferentiation rates.  These  data  indicate  that  gill  formation, 
brim  formation,  and  growth  rate  were  affected  similarly 
by  rearing  temperature  in  only  one  of  the  four  experiments 


(Experiment  III);  only  two  temperature  treatments  were 
tested  in  that  experiment. 

Larvae  typically  became  gilled  between  about  620-820 
^m  and  brimmed  between  about  710-850  nm,  with  no 
consistent  influence  of  rearing  temperature  or  feeding  fre- 
quency. Some  larvae  within  the  populations  were  fully 
gilled  and  brimmed  before  other  larvae  in  the  same  pop- 
ulation became  gilled,  indicating  much  individual  vari- 
ation in  rates  of  morphological  development  within  each 
temperature  treatment. 

All  but  seven  metamorphosed  individuals — out  of 
thousands  of  metamorphosed  snails  examined  in  these 
experiments — had  conspicuous  gills,  suggesting  that  most 
larvae  developed  gills  before  they  became  competent  to 
metamorphose.  The  seven  gill-less  juveniles  were  all  found 
at  29°C  (Experiments  I  and  II). 

Elevated  KCl  concentration  stimulates  metamorphosis 

Response  to  elevated  KCl  was  rapid.  Of  those  22-day- 
old  individuals  (22°C)  that  eventually  responded,  in- 
creasing KCl  concentrations  by  20  iruY/  induced  at  least 
90%  to  metamorphose  within  6  h  (Fig.  1).  Thus,  in  all 
subsequent  experiments,  larvae  were  exposed  to  elevated 
KCl  concentrations  for  6  h  to  assess  metamorphic  com- 
petence, defined  here  by  the  response  to  elevated  potas- 
sium. 

Effect  of  temperature  on  rates  of  becoming  competent  to 
metamorphose 

Despite  the  unpredictable  effects  of  rearing  temperature 
on  rates  of  growth  and  morphological  differentiation,  in- 
creasing larval  rearing  temperature  significantly  increased 
(P  <  0.001)  the  rates  at  which  larvae  became  competent 
to  metamorphose  in  all  experiments  (Tables  I,  III; 
Figs.  2,  3). 

Larval  shell  length  was  a  poor  indicator  of  whether  a 
larva  was  competent  to  metamorphose.  Although  com- 
petent larvae  were,  on  average,  significantly  larger  (P 
<  0.000 1 )  than  pre-competent  larvae  of  the  same  age  and 
rearing  history,  shell  lengths  of  competent  and  pre-com- 
petent larvae  overlapped  in  all  experiments,  as  exemplified 
by  Experiment  IV  (Fig.  4). 

Effect  of  temperature  on  the  maximum  length  oflan'al 
life  and  period  of  delayed  metamorphosis 

At  higher  temperatures,  larvae  consistently  exhibited 
"spontaneous"  metamorphosis  sooner  than  at  lower  tem- 
peratures (Table  IV  and  Fig.  5).  However,  average  growth 
rates  failed  to  predict  rates  of  spontaneous  metamorphosis 
within  a  population.  In  Experiment  I,  for  example,  larvae 
reared  at  20°C  or  25°C  grew  at  equivalent  rates  but  meta- 
morphosed faster  at  the  higher  temperature  (Fig.  5).  Even 


380 


R.  M.  ZIMMERMAN  AND  J.  A.  PECHENIR 


14       16 


18      20      22      24      26      28      30 

LARVAL  AGE  (DAYS  FROM  RELEASE) 


32       34       36       38 


Figure  5.  Maximum  length  of  larval  life  for  Crepidula  plana  maintained  in  glass  bowls,  acid-washed 
daily  (Experiment  I).  Each  point  represents  the  mean  of  three  replicates  (110  larvae  per  treatment).  Different 
letters  signify  larval  populations  differing  significantly  in  mean  growth  rates  (A  <  B  <  C). 


so,  individuals  exhibiting  faster  growth  within  a  temper- 
ature treatment  tended  to  metamorphose  sooner  than 
slower  growing  larvae  reared  at  the  same  temperature, 
confirming  previous  results  (Lima  and  Pechenik,  1985) 
(Fig.  6;  regression  analysis  of  log  growth  rate).  Within 
each  temperature,  faster  growing  individuals  also  tended 
to  metamorphose  at  larger  shell  lengths,  although  the  data 
do  show  considerable  scatter  (Fig.  7;  P  <  0.05  at  each 
temperature).  Individual  growth  rates  were  estimated  us- 
ing age  and  size  at  metamorphosis  (Lima  and  Pechenik, 
1985). 

Generally,  larvae  maintained  in  bowls  cleaned  only  ev- 
ery 48  h  metamorphosed  significantly  sooner  (P  <  0.05; 
/-test),  by  about  5-10  days,  than  larvae  maintained  in 
bowls  cleaned  every  24  h,  and  at  smaller  shell  lengths 
[smaller  by  about  1 00-300  ^m  (Zimmerman,  1989)].  This 
indicates  that  microbial  films  formed  over  48  h  could  trig- 
ger larvae  of  C.  plana  to  metamorphose,  supporting  pre- 
vious reports  (Lima,  1983). 

The  average  delay  period,  defined  here  by  the  difference 
(in  days)  between  (a)  mean  age  at  "spontaneous"  meta- 
morphosis in  bowls  cleaned  daily  and  (b)  when  50%  of  a 
larval  population  was  competent  to  metamorphose,  varied 
between  experiments,  and  was  markedly  altered  by  tem- 
perature only  in  Experiment  II  (Table  IV). 

Effect  of  salinity  on  rates  of  growth  and  morphological 
differentiation 

The  effects  of  salinity  on  growth  rate  differed  in  the 
two  experiments.  In  Experiment  V  (Table  I),  larvae  grew 
more  quickly  at  higher  salinities  (by  about  12  ^m  •  day"1 
for  each  salinity  increase  above  1 9  ppt).  In  the  three  lowest 
salinities  (4,  8,  and  14.5  ppt),  larvae  suffered  high  mortality 


(85-100%  at  4  and  8  ppt)  and  exhibited  no  detectable 
growth.  In  Experiment  VI,  salinity  significantly  affected 
mean  growth  rates,  but  not  as  dramatically  as  in  Exper- 
iment V,  and  not  in  direct  proportion  to  salinity.  Larvae 
reared  at  20  ppt  grew  significantly  faster  than  larvae  at  25 
ppt  in  Experiment  VI  (Tables  I.  III).  In  both  salinity  ex- 
periments, larvae  reared  in  full  strength  seawater  (either 
29  or  30  ppt)  grew  at  rates  comparable  to  those  of  larvae 
reared  under  comparable  conditions  (25°C,  full  strength 
seawater)  in  Experiments  I-IV  (Table  I). 

Salinity  over  the  range  of  20-30  ppt  had  negligible  ef- 
fects on  rates  of  gill  formation  (Tables  I,  V)  and  on  the 
shell  sizes  at  which  larvae  became  either  gilled  or 
brimmed.  Larvae  became  gilled  and  brimmed  at  shell  sizes 
between  628-728  /urn  and  699-790  ^m,  respectively,  re- 
gardless of  rearing  salinity.  However,  every  increase  in 
rearing  salinity  increased  rates  of  shell  brim  formation 
(Table  III).  The  pattern  of  significant  salinity  effects  on 
rates  of  growth  and  on  rates  of  gill  and  brim  formation 
(Table  III)  indicates  that  rates  of  growth  and  morpholog- 
ical differentiation  were  not  affected  similarly  by  changes 
in  salinity. 

The  relative  effect  of  salinity  on  rates  of  becoming 
competent  to  metamorphose  and  rates  of  growth 

Despite  the  erratic  influence  of  salinity  on  rates  of 
growth,  gill,  and  shell  brim  formation,  larvae  reared  at 
higher  salinities  typically  became  competent  to  meta- 
morphose sooner  than  those  reared  at  lower  salinities  in 
both  Experiments  V  and  VI  (Tables  I-III).  These  results 
suggest  that  changes  in  salinity  may  uncouple  rates  of 
growth,  rates  of  morphological  differentiation,  and  rates 
of  becoming  competent  to  metamorphose.  In  Experiment 


COMPETENCE  TO  METAMORPHOSE 


381 


41.1 


'- 


~ 


25 


20 


15 


: 


•i 


40 


60 


70 


80 


90 


INDIVIDUAL  GROWTH   RATE    CJJM-DAY      ) 


29°C 

* 

25°C 

O 

20°C 


Figure  6.  Maximum  length  of  larval  life  as  a  function  of  estimated  individual  growth  rate  (^rn-day  ') 
in  Experiment  IV  (r  =  0.74,  y  =  -  16.4(ln  x)  +  88.7).  Individual  growth  rates  were  estimated  from  the  size 
and  age  at  which  each  individual  underwent  spontaneous  metamorphosis  in  glass  bowls  that  were  cleaned 
daily.  Larvae  were  cultured  at  three  temperatures,  as  indicated  (n  =  64,  62.  and  63  larvae  per  treatment  at 
29°C,  25°C  and  20°C,  respectively). 


VI,  for  example,  larvae  grew  more  rapidly  at  20  ppt  than 
at  25  ppt,  but  took  longer  to  become  competent  at  the 
lower  salinity  (Table  III  and  Fig.  8).  Experiment  VI  was 
terminated  before  all  larvae  were  allowed  to  metamor- 
phose, so  calculation  of  age  and  size  at  metamorphosis 
was  not  possible. 

There  was  no  significant  difference  (P  >  0.05)  in  the 
percentage  of  larvae  induced  to  metamorphose  when  KC1 
concentrations  were  elevated  by  20  versus  23  mAl  at  20 
ppt.  Thus,  the  dilution  of  full  strength  seawater  to  make 
20  ppt  and  25  ppt  seawater  did  not  significantly  affect  the 
ability  of  KC1  to  induce  larval  metamorphosis. 


Discussion 

The  primary  goal  of  these  experiments  was  to  deter- 
mine, for  Crepidula  planu.  whether  changes  in  tempera- 
ture and  salinity  alter  rates  of  growth,  morphological  dif- 
ferentiation, and  the  onset  of  competence  equally.  We 
must  first  consider  the  effects  of  temperature  and  salinity 
on  each  of  these  three  components  of  development  in- 
dividually. 

Larvae  grew  significantly  faster  at  progressively  higher 
temperatures  (Table  I)  in  only  one  experiment  (Experi- 
ment III).  Lima  and  Pechenik  (1985)  also  found  an  in- 


1600 
1500 
1400 
1300 
1200 

i 

1100 
1000 


o  * 


30 


40 


50 


60 


70 


80 


90 


INDIVIDUAL  GROWTH  RATE  (UM  DAY   ) 


29"C 

* 

25°C 

O 

20°C 
A 


Figure  7.  Size  at  metamorphosis  as  a  function  of  individual  larval  growth  rate  (^m  -  day  ' )  (Experiment 
IV).  Faster  growing  larvae  tended  to  spontaneously  metamorphose  at  larger  shell  sizes  (P  <  0.05:  r  =  0.549 
at  29°C,  n  =  64;  r  =  0.512  at  25°C.  n  =  62;  r  =  0.51 1  at  20°C  n  =  63;  combined  r  =  0.249,  n  =  189). 
Growth  rates  were  estimated  from  size  and  age  at  spontaneous  metamorphosis.  Larvae  were  reared  at  three 
temperatures,  as  indicated. 


382 


K.  M.  ZIMMERMAN  AND  J.  A.  PECHENIK 


Table  V 

hv  nl  temperature  and  salinity  on  percent  changes  in  rates  of  shell  growth,  morphological  differentiation,  becoming  competent  to 
metamorphose,  and  spontaneous  metamorphosis  lor  nil  experiments 


Experiment 
number 

Temperature,  salinity 
(°C)           (ppt) 

Growth  rate 
(^m  -day"') 

1  •  (Time  to  50% 
competent)  ' 

1  •  (Time  to  50% 

gilled)  ' 

1  -(Time  to  50% 
bnmmedr1 

1  -(Time  to  50% 
spontaneously 
metamorphosed)"1 

I 

20°C.  30  ppt 











25°C,  30  ppt 

+  18% 

+  19% 

— 

— 

+21% 

29°C.  30  ppt 

-16% 

+48% 

— 

— 

+48% 

11 

20°C.  30  ppt 

— 

— 

— 

— 

— 

25°C.  30  ppt 

+  17% 

+  10% 

— 

— 

+  11% 

29  °C,  30  ppt 

+45% 

+35% 

— 

— 

+48% 

III 

25  °C.  30  ppt 

— 

— 

— 

— 

— 

29°C,  30  ppt 

+  14% 

+28% 

+6% 

+24% 

+  18% 

IV 

20°C,  30  ppt 

— 

— 

— 

— 

— 

25°C.  30  ppt 

+46% 

+32% 

+25% 

+34% 

+22% 

29°C,  30  ppt 

+38% 

+41% 

+38% 

+38% 

+29% 

V 

25°C.  8  ppt 

— 

— 

— 

— 

— 

25°C.  14.5  ppt 

+  12% 

+20% 

— 

— 

— 

25°C,  19  ppt 

+233% 

+3.3% 

— 

— 

— 

25°C,  25  ppt 

+  334% 

+  10% 

— 

— 

— 

25°C.  29  ppt 

+438% 

+37% 

— 

— 

— 

VI 

25°C,  20  ppt 

— 

— 

— 

— 

— 

25°C,  25  ppt 

-9% 

+20% 

+5% 

+4% 

— 

25°C,  30  ppt 

+  12% 

+35% 

+11% 

+6% 

— 

Each  percent  change  was  calculated  relative  to  the  lowest  temperature,  salinity  treatment  in  a  particular  experiment. 


consistent  effect  of  temperature  on  larval  growth  rate  for 
larvae  of  C.  plana  fed  T-ISO,  and  the  larval  growth  rates 
reported  here  for  C.  plana  are  generally  comparable  to 
those  previously  reported  by  Lima  and  Pechenik  (1985) 
for  larvae  reared  at  identical  food  concentrations  and 
temperatures.  In  our  Experiment  III,  however,  larvae 
reared  at  29°C  and  25°C  grew  1.5-2.0  times  faster  than 
those  reared  by  Lima  and  Pechenik  (1985)  under  the  same 
conditions.  Lima  and  Pechenik  (1985)  reported  compar- 
ably high  larval  growth  rates  (exceeding  50  /im-day"') 
for  C.  plana  reared  at  25  °C  and  29 °C  on  a  different  naked 
flagellate,  Isochrysis  galbana  (clone  ISO).  Larvae  of  the 
congener  C.  fomicata  also  grow  at  rates  exceeding  50 
nm  •  day  ',  at  temperatures  above  24°C  (Lucas  and  Cost- 
low,  1979;  Pechenik,  1984;  Pechenik  and  Lima,  1984). 

Salinity  also  influenced  larval  growth  rates,  although 
the  effects  were  often  inconsistent  between  the  two  ex- 
periments (Tables  I,  III).  Larvae  grew  faster  at  progres- 
sively higher  salinities  in  Experiment  V,  but  not  in  Ex- 
periment VI  (Tables  I,  III).  As  with  other  molluscan  larvae, 
including  the  congener  C.  fomicata  (Davis,  1958;  Davis 
and  Ansell,  1962;  Davis  and  Calabrese,  1964;  Scheltema. 
1965;  Calabrese  and  Rhodes,  1974;  Robert  el  ai.  1988; 
Hisi'/fl/.,  1989),  those  of  C.  plana  grew  poorly  at  salinities 
below  about  20  ppt. 


The  influence  of  temperature  on  rates  of  morphological 
differentiation  also  varied  from  one  experiment  to  the 
next  (Table  III).  This  contrasts  with  results  reported  for 
C.  fomicata  by  Pechenik  and  Lima  (1984)  and  Pechenik 
(1984),  who  found  that  larvae  always  tended  to  develop 
gills  and  shell  brims  more  rapidly  at  higher  temperatures. 
We  have  no  way  of  knowing  whether  the  inter-experiment 
variation  we  report  for  C.  plana  reflects  genetic  differences 
in  the  larval  populations  used,  subtle  differences  in  rearing 
conditions  among  experiments,  or  differences  in  the 
physiological  history  of  the  adults  that  released  the  larvae 
used  in  these  experiments  (Bayne  ct  ai.  1975).  Increases 
in  salinity  did  not  predictably  alter  rates  of  gill  formation 
in  Experiment  VI  (Table  III),  but  shell  brims  formed  more 
rapidly  at  higher  salinities. 

As  reported  previously  for  larvae  of  C.  plana  and  C. 
fornicala  (Pechenik  and  Lima,  1984;  Lima  and  Pechenik, 
1985),  and  for  larvae  of  the  blue  mussel  AI.  edidis  (Pech- 
enik ct  al.,  1990),  temperature  apparently  altered  rates  of 
growth  and  morphological  development  to  different  de- 
grees. In  our  studies,  this  is  suggested  by  the  fact  that 
larvae  tended  to  develop  shell  brims  and  visible  gill  fila- 
ments at  different  sizes  when  reared  at  different  temper- 
atures. For  example,  in  Experiment  I,  larvae  formed  vis- 
ible gill  filaments  on  average  between  681  and  721  ^m. 


COMPETENCE  TO  METAMORPHOSE 


383 


30  PPT 


10    11    12    13    14    15    16    17    19    19   20   21    22 


LARVAL  AGE  (DAYS  FPOM  RELEASE) 

Figure  8.  Influence  of  salinity  on  the  rate  at  which  larvae  became  competent  to  metamorphose  in 
Experiment  VI.  Each  point  represents  the  mean  percentage  of  larvae  competent  in  three  howls,  with  20-23 
larvae  tested  per  bowl.  Vertical  bars  represent  one  SD  about  the  mean.  Different  letters  represent  larval 
populations  with  different  mean  growth  rates  (A  <  B  <  C). 


between  675  and  817  ^m,  and  between  742  and  786  ^m, 
at  29°C,  25°C.  and  20°C,  respectively.  Rates  of  shell 
growth  would  have  to  be  altered  by  temperature  in  exact 
proportion  to  any  changes  in  rates  of  morphological  de- 
velopment if  larvae  are  to  form  gills  and  shell  brims  at 
comparable  average  sizes  in  all  rearing  conditions  (Pech- 
enik  and  Lima,  1984;  Pechenik  el  ai.  1990). 

Rates  of  shell  growth  and  morphological  differentiation 
were  also  affected  to  different  degrees  by  salinity.  For  ex- 
ample, in  Experiment  VI,  larvae  grew  significantly  faster 
at  20  ppt  than  at  25  ppt,  but  the  salinity  decrease  did  not 
affect  rate  of  gill  formation.  Indeed,  rate  of  gill  formation 
was  not  affected  by  salinity  over  the  range  tested.  In  con- 
trast, shell  brims  formed  faster  at  the  higher  salinity. 

Despite  the  generally  unpredictable  effects  of  temper- 
ature on  rates  of  larval  growth  and  morphological  differ- 
entiation both  among  and,  often,  within  experiments,  the 
influence  of  temperature  on  the  rates  at  which  larvae  be- 
came competent  to  metamorphose  was  remarkably  con- 
sistent among  all  four  experiments;  larvae  always  became 
competent  to  metamorphose  faster  when  reared  at  higher 
temperatures  (Table  III  and  Fig.  3).  Rates  of  becoming 
competent  to  metamorphose  were  clearly  uncoupled  from 
rates  of  morphological  differentiation  and  shell  growth. 
In  Experiment  I,  for  example,  larvae  reared  at  29 °C  be- 
came competent  significantly  sooner  (and  often  at  smaller 
sizes)  than  larvae  reared  at  20°C  or  25°C,  despite  signif- 
icantly slower  average  growth  for  larvae  reared  at  the 
higher  temperature  (Figs.  2,  4).  In  addition,  larvae  reared 
at  25 °C  became  competent  significantly  sooner  than  lar- 
vae at  20°C,  even  though  these  larvae  did  not  grow  at 
significantly  different  rates  at  the  two  temperatures. 

The  same  was  true  of  the  experiments  (V  and  VI)  ex- 


amining the  influence  of  salinity.  Here  again,  larvae  reared 
at  higher  salinities  generally  became  competent  faster, 
while  rates  of  growth  and  morphological  differentiation 
were  not  so  predictably  affected.  For  example,  in  Exper- 
iment VI,  larvae  reared  at  20  ppt  grew  significantly  faster 
than  larvae  at  25  ppt,  but  those  larvae  reared  at  20  ppt 
became  competent  at  slower  rates  (Table  III  and  Fig.  8). 
The  influence  of  temperature  or  salinity  on  the  amount 
of  time  required  for  larvae  in  a  population  to  become 
competent  clearly  cannot  be  predicted  from  the  effects  of 
environmental  change  on  rates  of  growth  (Table  V).  In- 
dividual competence  also  cannot  be  predicted  on  the  basis 
of  shell  length  (Fig.  4)  or  the  presence  of  a  shell  brim  or 
visible  gill  filaments;  at  least  some  gill-less  larvae  were 
induced  to  metamorphose  by  elevating  KG  concentration 
(at  29°C,  Experiments  I  and  II).  Also,  in  every  experiment, 
at  every  temperature,  some  larvae  without  shell  brims 
could  be  induced  to  metamorphose.  Similarly,  neither 
shell  size  nor  morphological  indicators  were  adequate 
predictors  of  whether  individual  blue  mussel  larvae  would 
or  would  not  attach  to  filamentous  substrates  in  the  lab- 
oratory (Eyster  and  Pechenik,  1987),  or  when  oyster  larvae 
(Crassostrea  gigas)  would  exhibit  settlement  behavior  in 
response  to  L-DOPA  (Coon  el  al,  1990). 

Variation  in  the  rates  at  which  individuals  became 
competent  to  metamorphose  within  treatments  (as  in  Figs. 
2  and  8)  may  be  a  natural  phenomenon  that  encourages 
larvae  released  from  an  individual  female  to  metamor- 
phose at  different  times,  likely  increasing  the  spread  of 
siblings  among  different  populations  (Strathmann,  1974; 
Hadfield,  1977)  and  minimizing  their  competition  for 
food  and  space  as  juveniles. 

In  our  experiments,   larvae  consistently  underwent 


384 


K.  M.  ZIMMERMAN  AND  J.  A.  PECHENIK 


32 
30 

28 
26 
24 

22 

20 
18 

16 
14 


DAYS  TO  50  J  LARVAL  POPULATION  COMPETENT  TO  METAMORPHOSE 

Figure  9.  Influence  of  temperature  on  the  relationship  between  the  maximum  length  of  larval  lite  and 
the  time  required  for  50%  of  a  population  to  become  competent.  Larvae  ofCrepidulu  plana  were  reared  at 
three  temperatures,  as  indicated.  For  each  temperature,  different  bars  represent  data  from  different  experiments. 


s 

-V 

i? 

1 

„ 

I          \          \ 

19                  21                  23 

"spontaneous"  metamorphosis  sooner  at  higher  temper- 
atures (Fig.  5  and  Table  IV).  This  phenomenon  of  mol- 
luscan  larvae  metamorphosing  sooner  in  warmer  tem- 
peratures has  been  noted  previously  (Loosanoff,  1959: 
Davis  and  Ansell,  1962;  Davis  and  Calabrese,  1964; 
Bayne.  1965;  Pechenik,  1984;  Pechenik  and  Lima,  1984; 
Lima  and  Pechenik,  1985).  This  relationship  is  consistent 
with  the  hypothesis  that  the  timing  of  spontaneous  meta- 
morphosis is  determined  by  the  rate  at  which  larvae  pro- 
gress through  a  developmental  program  with  a  fixed  end- 
point  (Pechenik,  1980,  1984;  Pechenik  and  Lima,  1984); 
the  endpoint  could  be  determined  by  some  endogenous 
controlling  factor  or,  as  suggested  recently  by  Coon  et  al. 
( 1990),  could  reflect  a  gradually  increasing  sensitivity  of 
receptors  for  an  external  chemical  cue  present  naturally 
in  extremely  low  concentrations.  Although  mean  growth 
rates  were  not  adequate  indicators  of  the  rates  at  which 
larvae  within  a  population  would  undergo  spontaneous 
metamorphosis  (Fig.  5),  faster  growing  individuals  did 
tend  to  exhibit  spontaneous  metamorphosis  sooner  than 
slower  growing  individuals  (Fig.  6).  These  results  are  sim- 
ilar to  those  reported  previously  for  this  species  (Lima 
and  Pechenik,  1985),  for  the  congener  C.fornicata  (Pech- 
enik, 1984;  Pechenik  and  Lima,  1984),  and  for  the  bivalves 
Mercenaria  merccnaria  (Loosanoff,  1959)  and  M.  cctulis 
(Beaumont  and  Budd,  1982). 

Despite  the  pronounced  influence  of  temperature  on 
rates  of  becoming  competent  and  rates  of  spontaneous 
metamorphosis  (Tables  III,  IV),  temperature  had  a  minor 
influence  on  the  length  of  time  that  larvae  of  C.  plana 


delayed  metamorphosis  in  frequently  cleaned  glass  bowls 
in  all  but  one  experiment  (Table  IV  and  Fig.  9).  Only  in 
Experiment  II  did  temperature  affect  delay  period  by  more 
than  one  or  two  days  (Table  IV).  Thus,  although  larvae 
of  C.  plana  will  have  a  longer  pre-competent  period  at 
lower  temperatures,  the  capacity  for  delaying  metamor- 
phosis in  the  absence  of  suitable  substrate  may  be  affected 
to  a  much  lesser  degree. 

In  our  experiments,  larvae  metamorphosed  "sponta- 
neously" about  3.5-12  days  after  becoming  competent, 
with  most  delay  periods  lying  between  about  5  and  7  days 
(Table  IV).  These  data  are  comparable  to  earlier  laboratory 
estimates  of  delay  potential  for  larvae  of  this  species  reared 
at  comparable  temperatures  (Lima  and  Pechenik,  1985: 
their  Table  II),  and  seem  to  confirm  the  reduced  capacity 
of  C.  plana  for  postponing  metamorphosis  relative  to  that 
exhibited  by  larvae  of  C.  fornicata;  the  maximum  delay 
period  of  about  20-30  days  suggested  for  C.  fornicata 
(Pechenik,  1984)  is  only  an  estimate,  however,  and  has 
not  yet  been  confirmed.  The  estimates  of  delay  potential 
for  C.  plana  reared  at  different  temperatures  given  by  Lima 
and  Pechenik  (1985)  were  based  on  the  assumption  that 
competence  is  attained  at  a  particular  shell  length.  The 
good  agreement  between  their  estimates  and  our  more 
direct  determinations  suggest  that  although  individual 
larvae  clearly  do  not  become  competent  to  metamorphose 
at  a  particular  size,  the  simplifying  assumption  of  length- 
related  competence  may  permit  adequate  predictions  at 
the  population  level. 

Clearly,  the  various  aspects  of  morphological   and 


COMPETENCE  TO  METAMORPHOSE 


385 


physiological  development  are  affected  to  different  degrees 
in  C.  plana  by  temperature  and  salinity  changes.  In  only 
one  experiment  (Experiment  III)  did  increased  rearing 
temperature  significantly  increase  all  components  of  de- 
velopmental rate  that  were  monitored:  shell  and  tissue 
growth,  timing  of  gill  differentiation  and  shell  brim  de- 
velopment, and  onset  of  metamorphic  competence.  The 
likely  impact  of  environmental  factors  on  larval  dispersal 
periods  therefore  cannot  be  estimated  from  data  on  rates 
of  growth  or  morphological  development,  but  clearly  must 
be  determined  directly.  Our  data  suggest  that  changes  in 
temperature  and  salinity  will  have  a  more  consistent  in- 
fluence on  duration  of  pre-competent  and  competent  pe- 
riods of  development  than  on  either  rates  of  shell  growth 
or  rates  of  morphological  differentiation. 

Acknowledgments 

This  research  was  completed  in  partial  fulfillment  of 
the  requirements  for  the  degree  of  Master  of  Science  to 
K.  M.  Zimmerman.  Summer  support  for  K.  M.  Zim- 
merman was  provided  by  NSF  Grant  OCE-8500857  to 
J.  A.  Pechenik.  We  thank  Durwood  Marshall  for  advising 
on  analysis  of  covariance  and  Carol  Valente  and  Valerie 
Ricciardone  for  typing  the  manuscript.  The  manuscript 
has  benefited  from  the  suggestions  of  two  anonymous  re- 
viewers. 

Literature  Cited 

Bayne,  B.  L.  1964.  The  responses  of  the  larvae  ofMylilus  edulis  L.  to 
light  and  to  gravity.  Oikos  15:  162-174. 

Bayne,  B.  L.  1%5.  Growth  and  delay  of  metamorphosis  of  the  larvae 
ofMytilw;  edulis  L.  Ophelia  2(1):  1-47. 

Bayne,  B.  L.  1971.  Some  morphological  changes  that  occur  at  the 
metamorphosis  of  the  larvae  of  Mylilus  edulis.  Pp.  259-280  in  Fourth 
European  Marine  Biology  Symposium.  D.  J.  Crisp,  ed.  Cambridge 
University  Press.  Cambridge. 

Bayne,  B.  L.,  P.  A.  Gabbott,  and  J.  Widdovts.  1975.  Some  effects  of 
stress  in  the  adult  on  the  eggs  and  larvae  of  Mytilus  edulis  L.  J  Mar. 
Biol.  Assoc.  V.K.  55:  675-689. 

Beaumont,  A.  R.,  and  M.  D.  Budd.  1982.  Delayed  growth  of  mussel 
(Mytilus  edulis)  and  scallop  (Pecten  maximus)  veligers  at  low  tem- 
peratures. Mar.  Biol.  71:  97-100. 

Butman,  C.  A.,  J.  P.  Grassle,  and  C.  M.  Webb.  1988.  Substrate  choices 
made  by  marine  larvae  settling  in  still  water  and  in  a  flume  flow. 
Nature  333:  771-773. 

Calabrese,  A.,  and  E.  \V.  Rhodes.  1974.  Culture  of  Mu/inia  laleralis 
and  Crepidula  fornicata  embryos  and  larvae  for  studies  of  pollution 
effects.  Thalassia  Jugoslav.  10(1/2):  89-102. 

Chia.  F.  S.  1978.  Perspectives:  settlement  and  metamorphosis  of  marine 
invertebrate  larvae.  Pp.  283-285  in  Settlement  ami  Metamorphosis 
of  Marine  Invertebrate  Larvae.  F.  S.  Chia  and  M.  E.  Rice,  eds.  Elsevier, 
New  York. 

Coon,  S.  L.,  D.  B.  Bonar,  and  R.  M.  Weiner.  1985.  Induction  of  set- 
tlement and  metamorphosis  of  the  pacific  oyster.  Crassostrea  gigas 
(Thunberg),  by  L-DOPA  and  catecholamines.  J.  Exp.  Mar.  Biol 
Ecol.  94:211-221. 

Coon,  S.  L.,  W.  K.  Fitt,  and  D.  B.  Bonar.  1990.     Competency  and  delay 


of  metamorphosis  in  the  Pacific  oyster  Crassostrea  gigas.  Mar.  Biol. 

106:  379-387. 

Crisp,  D.  J.  1974.     Factors  influencing  the  settlement  of  marine  inver- 
tebrate larvae.  Pp.  177-265  in  Chemoreception  in  Marine  Organisms. 

P.  T.  Grant  and  A.  M.  Mackie,  eds.  Academic  Press,  New  York. 
Davis,  H.  C.  1958.     Survival  and  growth  of  clam  and  oyster  larvae  at 

different  salinities.  Biol  Bull  114(3):  296-307. 
Davis,  H.  C.,  and  A.  D.  Ansell.  1962.     Survival  and  growth  of  larvae 

of  the  european  oyster,  O  edulis.  at  lowered  salinities.  Biol.  Bull. 

122:  33-39. 
Davis,  H.  C.,  and  A.  Calabrese.  1964.     Combined  effects  of  temperature 

and  salinity  on  development  of  eggs  and  growth  of  larvae  of  M.  mer- 

cenaria  and  C.  virninica.  U  S  Fish  Wild.  Fish.  Bull.  63(3):  643- 

655. 
Draper,  N.  R.,  and  H.  Smith.  1981.     Applied  Regression  Analysis.  John 

Wiley  and  Sons.  Inc..  New  York.  567  pp. 
Eyster,  L.  S.,  and  J.  A.  Pechenik.  1987.     Attachment  ofMylilus  edulis 

L.  larvae  on  algal  and  hyssal  filaments  is  enhanced  by  water  agitation. 

/  Exp.  Mar  Biol  Kail.  114:  99-1 10. 
Fitt,  W .  K.,  S.  L.  Coon,  M.  Walch,  R.  M.  Weiner,  R.  R.  Colwell,  and 

D.  B.  Bonar.  1990.     Settlement  behavior  and  metamorphosis  of 

oyster  larvae  (Crassostrea  gigas)  in  response  to  bacterial  supernatants. 

Mar.  Biol  106:  389-394. 
Hadfield,  M.  G.  1977.     Chemical  interactions  in  larval  settling  of  a 

marine  gastropod.  Pp  403-4 1 3  in  Marine  Natural  Products  Chemistry. 

D.  J.  Faulkner  &  W.  H.  Fenical.  eds.  Plenum  Publishing  Corp.,  New 

York. 
Hadfield,  M.  G.  1978.     Metamorphosis  in  marine  molluscan  larvae:  an 

analysis  of  stimulis  and  response.  Pp.  165-175  in  Settlement  and 

Metamorphosis  of  Marine  Invertebrate  Larvae.  F.  S.  Chia  and  M.  E. 

Rice,  eds.  Elsevier,  New  York. 
Hickman,  R.  W.,  and  LL.  D.  Gruffydd.  1971.     The  histology  of  the 

larva  ofOstrea  edit/is  during  metamorphosis.  Pp.  28 1-294  in  Fourth 

European  Marine  Biology  Symposium.  D.  J.  Crisp,  ed.  Cambridge 

University  Press,  Cambridge. 
His,  E.,  R.  Robert,  and  A.  Dinct.  1989.     Combined  effects  of  temperature 

and  salinity  on  fed  and  starved  larvae  of  the  Mediterranean  mussel 

Mytilus  galloprovincialis  and  the  Japanese  oyster  Crassostrea  gigas. 

Mar.  Biol  100:  455-463. 
Jackson,  G.  A.,  and  R.  R.  Strathmann.  1981.     Larval  mortality  from 

offshore  mixing  as  a  link  between  precompetent  and  competent  pe- 
riods of  development.  .!w.  Nat.  118(1):  16-26. 
Kirchman,  D.,  S.  Graham,  D.  Reish,  and  R.  Mitchell.  1982.     Bacteria 

induce  settlement  and  metamorphosis  of  Janua  (Dexiospira)  brasil- 

iensisGrube  (Polychaeta:Spirorbidae).  J.  Exp  Mar.  Biol.  Ecol.  56: 

153-163. 
Kleinbaum,  D.  G.,  L.  L.  Kupper,  and  K.  E.  Muller.  1988.     Applied 

Regression  Analysis  and  Other  Multivariate  Methods.  PWS-Kent. 

Boston.  718  pp. 
Lima,  G.  M.  1983.     The  relationship  of  temperature  on  growth  rates 

and  the  length  of  larval  life  of  the  gastropod.  Crepidula  plana  Say. 

Master's  Thesis,  Tufts  University.  Medford.  MA.  1 15  pp. 
Lima,  G.  M.,  and  J.  A.  Pechenik.  1985.     The  influence  of  temperature 

on  growth  rate  and  length  of  larval  life  of  the  gastropod.  Crepidula 

plana  (Say).  J.  Exp.  Mar.  Biol.  Eeol.  90:  55-71. 
Loosanoff,  V.  L.  1959.     The  size  and  shape  of  metamorphosing  larvae 

of  I  'emu  (Mereenaria)  mercenarm  grown  at  different  temperatures. 

Biol.  Bull  117:  308-318. 
Lucas,  J.  S.,  and  J.  D.  Costlow,  Jr.  1979.     Effects  of  various  temperature 

cycles  on  the  larval  development  of  the  gastropod  mollusc  Crepidula 

fornicata.  Mar.  Biol  51:  111-117. 
McGee,  B.  L.,  and  N.  M.  Target!.  1989.     Larval  habitat  selection  of 

Crepidula  (L.)  and  its  effect  on  adult  distribution  patterns.  J.  Exp. 

Mar.  Biol.  Ecol.  131:  195-214. 
Meadows,  P.  S.,  and  J.  I.  Campbell.  1972.     Habitat  selection  by  aquatic 


386 


K.  M.  ZIMMERMAN  AND  J.  A.  PECHENIK 


invertebrates.  Pp.  27 1  -36 1  in  Advances  in  Marine  Biology,  F.  S.  Rus- 
sell and  M.  Yonge,  eds.  Academic  Press,  New  York. 

Miller.  S.  E.  and  Hadfield,  M.  G.  1986.  Ontogeny  of  phototaxis  and 
metamorphic  competence  in  larvae  of  the  nudihranch  Phestilla  si- 
bogae  Bergh  (Gastropoda:Opisthobranchia).  J.  Exp  Mar.  Biol.  Ecol. 
97:95-112. 

Pechenik,  J.  A.  1980.  Growth  and  energy  balance  during  the  larval 
lives  of  three  prosobranch  gastropods.  J.  Exp.  Mar.  Biol.  Ecol.  44: 
1-28. 

Pechenik,  J.  A.  1984.  The  relationship  between  temperature,  growth 
rate,  and  duration  of  planktonic  life  for  larvae  of  the  gastropod  Cre- 
pidula Jormcala  (L.).  J  Exp.  Mar.  Biol.  Ecol.  74:  241-257. 

Pechenik,  J.  A.,  and  N.  S.  Fisher.  1979.  Feeding,  assimilation  and 
growth  of  mud  snail  larvae,  Nassarius  obsoletus  (Say),  on  three  dif- 
ferent algal  diets.  J.  Exp.  Mar.  Biol.  Ecol.  38:  57-80. 

Pechenik,  J.  A.,  and  G.  M.  Lima.  1984.  Relationship  between  growth, 
differentiation,  and  length  of  larval  life  for  individually  reared  larvae 
of  the  marine  gastropod,  Crepidula  fomicata.  Biol.  Bull.  166:  537- 
549. 

Pechenik,  J.  A.,  and  \V.  D.  Heyman.  1987.  Using  K.CI  to  determine 
size  at  competence  for  larvae  of  the  marine  gastropod  Crepidula  for- 
mcaia  (L.).  J.  Exp.  Mar.  Biol.  Ecol.  112:  27-38. 

Pechenik,  J.  A.,  L.  S.  Eyster,  J.  Widdows,  and  B.  L.  Bayne.  1990.  The 
influence  of  food  concentration  and  temperature  on  growth  and 
morphological  differentiation  of  blue  mussel  Mytilux  edulis  L.  larvae. 
J  Exp.  Mar.  Biol.  Ecol.  136:  47-63. 

Robert.  R.,  E.  His,  and  A.  Dinet.  1988.  Combined  effects  of  temperature 
and  salinity  on  fed  and  starved  larvae  of  the  European  flat  oyster 
Oslrea  edulis  Mar.  Biol.  97:  95-100. 

Scheltema,  R.  S.  1965.     The  relationship  of  salinity  to  larval  survival 


and  development  in  Nassarius  obsok'lus  (Gastropoda).  Biol.  Bull. 
65:  340-354. 

Scheltema,  R.  S.  1974.  Biological  interactions  determining  larval  set- 
tlement of  marine  invertebrates.  Thalassia  Jugslav.  10(1/2):  263- 
296. 

Scheltema,  R.  S.  1978.  On  the  relationship  between  dispersal  of  pelagic 
veliger  larvae  and  the  evolution  of  marine  prosobranch  gastropods. 
Pp.  303-322  in  Marine  Organisms.  B.  Battaglia  and  J.  A.  Beardmore, 
eds.  Plenum  Publishing.  New  York. 

SPSS,  Inc.  1988.  SPSS-X  User's  Guide.  Third  Edition.  SPSS.  Inc.. 
Chicago.  1072  pp. 

Strathmann.  R.  1974.  The  spread  of  sibling  larvae  of  sedentary  marine 
invertebrates.  Am.  Nat.  108(959):  117-123. 

Switzer-Dunlap,  M.,  and  M.  G.  Hadfield.  1977.  Observations  on  de- 
velopment, larval  growth  and  metamorphosis  of  four  species  of 
Aplysiidae  (Gastropoda:  Opisthobranchia)  in  laboratory  culture.  J. 
Exp.  Mar.  Biol.  Ecol.  29:  245-261. 

Weiner,  R.  M.,  M.  Walch,  M.  P.  Labare,  D.  B.  Bonar,  and  R.  R.  Colwell. 
1989.  Effect  of  biofilms  of  the  marine  bacterium  Alteromonas  col- 
welliana  (LST)  on  set  of  the  oysters  Crassostrea  gigas  (Thunberg,  1793) 
and  C.  \-irxinica  (Gmelm,  1 79 1 ).  /  Shellfish  Res.  8(  1 ):  117-1 23. 

YooL,  A.  J.,  S.  M.  Grau,  M.  G.  Hadfield,  R.  A.  Jensen,  D.  A.  Markell. 
and  D.  E.  Morse.  1986.  Excess  potassium  induces  larval  metamor- 
phosis in  four  marine  invertebrate  species.  Biol.  Bull.  170:  255-266. 

Zimmerman.  K.  M.  1989.  Differential  effects  of  temperature  and  salinity 
on  the  growth  and  differentiation  of  larvae  of  the  marine  gastropod 
Crepidula  plana.  Master's  Thesis,  Tufts  University.  Medford.  MA. 
89pp. 


Reference:  Biol.  Bull  180:  387-393.  (June,  1991) 


Predation  Risk  and  Avoidance  Behavior 
in  Two  Freshwater  Snails 

JAMES  E.  ALEXANDER,  JR.1  AND  ALAN  P.  COVICH 

Department  of  Zoology.  University  of  Oklahoma,  Norman,  Oklahoma  73019 


Abstract.  We  examined  the  predator  avoidance  behav- 
iors of  two  common  freshwater  snails,  Physella  virgata 
and  Planorbella  trivolvis,  to  the  crayfish  Procambams 
simulans.  In  response  to  crayfish  predation,  the  snails 
crawled  above  the  waterline  for  several  hours,  then  re- 
turned to  the  water.  A  significant  size-dependent  rela- 
tionship existed  between  crawlout  (vertical  migration 
above  the  waterline)  and  vulnerability  to  predation.  All 
observed  size  classes  of  P.  virgata,  and  small  P.  trivolvis, 
were  vulnerable  and  crawled  out  in  response  to  crayfish 
predation.  Large,  invulnerable  P.  trivolvis  did  not  display 
any  overt  avoidance  behavior,  but  relied  instead  on  strong 
shell  architecture  for  defense.  We  suggest  that,  in  these 
species,  crawling  above  the  waterline  reduces  the  proba- 
bility of  an  encounter  between  vulnerable  thin-shelled 
snails  and  crayfish.  This  behavior  is  an  adaptive  response 
to  predation. 

Introduction 

Predation  is  an  important  cause  of  evolutionary  change 
in  many  prey  taxa  (Vermeij  and  Covich,  1978;  Vermeij, 
1982a,  b).  Predators  influence  their  prey  populations  in 
various  ways;  one  aspect  of  predation  in  freshwater  sys- 
tems that  is  receiving  increasing  attention  is  the  behavioral 
interactions  that  occur  between  predator  and  prey  (Pec- 
karsky,  1984;  Sih,  1984).  The  relative  impact  of  inverte- 
brate predators  on  freshwater  snails,  and  the  responses  of 
the  snails  to  their  predators  have  frequently  been  studied 
(Townsend  and  McCarthy,  1980;  Covich,  1981;  Brown 
and  DeVries,  1985;  Lodge  et  at..  1987;  Brown  and  Strouse, 


Received  2  November  1990;  accepted  19  March  1991. 

'  To  whom  communications  should  be  sent.  Present  address:  De- 
partment of  Biology.  Box  19498,  The  University  of  Texas  at  Arlington, 
Arlington,  TX  76019. 


1988;  Crowl  and  Covich,  1990;  Crowl,  1990;  Hanson  et 
a/..  1 990;  Kesler  and  Munns,  1 990;  Alexander  and  Covich, 
1991).  Freshwater  snails  exhibit  predator  avoidance 
mechanisms,  such  as  burying  into  substrata,  and  crawling 
into  vegetation  or  above  the  waterline  (Snyder,  1967; 
Townsend  and  McCarthy,  1980;  Alexander  and  Covich, 
1991). 

Comparative  studies  on  a  variety  of  animals  have  shown 
that  closely  related  or  co-occurring  species  may  respond 
differently  to  a  predator.  In  other  situations,  juveniles  or 
smaller  individuals  that  are  vulnerable  to  predators  show 
stronger  antipredator  responses  than  larger,  older,  or  other, 
relatively  less  vulnerable  prey  (Stein,  1977;  Schmitt,  1982; 
Sih.  1982,  1986;  Werner  and  Hall,  1988).  In  these  studies, 
prey  appear  to  assess  the  tradeoffs  between  predation  risk 
and  foraging  for  food;  i.e..  the  vulnerable  species  or  size 
classes  forage  in  different  habitats,  or  at  different  times, 
than  the  invulnerable  prey.  Comparative  studies,  by  re- 
vealing the  variety  and  relative  effectiveness  of  antipreda- 
tor responses,  help  to  elucidate  the  adaptive  nature  of  a 
response.  In  this  paper,  we  describe  the  predator  avoidance 
response  of  two  common,  co-occurring  freshwater  snail 
species,  Physella  virgata  (Pulmonata,  Physidae,  Fig.  1A) 
and  Planorbella  trivolvis  (Pulmonata,  Planorbidae,  Fig. 
IB,  C),  to  their  predator,  the  crayfish  Procambams  simu- 
lans (Decapoda,  Astacidae).  In  another  paper  (Alexander 
and  Covich,  1 99 1 ),  we  demonstrated  that  Physella  virgata 
performs  a  chemically  mediated  predator  avoidance  be- 
havior (crawling  above  the  waterline  for  a  minimum  of 
2  h)  in  response  to  an  actively  foraging  crayfish  predator. 
Physella  virgata  appears  to  react  to  chemicals  emanating 
from  crayfish  and  from  injured  conspecifics.  In  this  study, 
we  demonstrate  a  size-dependent  avoidance  response  that 
corresponds  to  the  relative  vulnerability  of  a  snail  to  cray- 
fish predation. 


387 


388 


J.  E.  ALEXANDER,  JR.  AND  A.  P.  COVICH 


A 


2mm 


Figure  1.     The  shell  morphology  of  Plmclla  viruuta  (A)  and  Planor- 
bella  tri\-olvis  (B  and  C).  The  size  bar  is  2  mm. 


Materials  and  Methods 

Study  site  and  general  methods 

The  snails  and  crayfish  used  in  this  study  were  collected 
from  Oliver  Wildlife  Preserve  (Norman,  Oklahoma). 
Oliver  Wildlife  Preserve  is  a  forested  area  on  the  South 
Canadian  River  floodplain  that  is  inundated  periodically 
by  runoff  and  heavy  spring  rains.  The  middle  third  of  the 
preserve  typically  remains  under  water  throughout  the 
late  winter  to  early  summer  months  (December  to  June) 
and  supports  large  populations  of  P.  virgata.  P.  trivolvis, 
and  P.  simulans  (Alexander.  1987).  Woody  debris  in 
Oliver  Wildlife  Preserve  provide  abundant  substrata  onto 
which  the  snails  migrate  to  avoid  predators;  snails  were 
observed  above  the  waterline  throughout  the  year  at  Oliver 
Wildlife  Preserve  and  at  other  sites  (pers.  obs.). 

Laboratory  experiments  were  conducted  at  night,  in 
darkness,  simulating  the  natural  conditions  under  which 
crayfish  are  most  active.  No  substratum  was  included  in 
these  experiments.  For  the  handling  time  and  ingestion 
probability  experiments  (Experiment  1).  where  crayfish 
and  snails  were  under  continuous  observation,  low  inten- 
sity red  light  was  used  to  facilitate  observations.  In  the 
second  experiment,  low  intensity  white  light  was  used 
briefly  to  record  observations.  When  not  used  in  experi- 
ments, snails  were  maintained  in  40-80-1  aquaria  and  fed 
commercial  fish  food  (TetraMin)  and  lettuce  ad  libitum. 
Crayfish  were  housed  individually  in  4-1  plastic  containers 
and  fed  fish  food  pellets  and  lettuce  ad  libitum.  Crayfish 
were  starved  for  at  least  24  h  prior  to  the  start  of  the 
experiments. 

Experiment  1:  differential  vulnerability  of 
Physella  and  Planorbella 

This  experiment  was  aimed  at  examining  the  ability  of 
P.  simulans  to  handle  and  ingest  different  size  classes  of 


P.  virgata  and  P.  trivolvis.  A  10-1  aquarium  was  placed  so 
that  the  actions  of  the  crayfish  and  snails  could  be  observed 
under  low  intensity  red  light  illumination,  regardless  of 
their  position  in  the  aquarium.  The  snails  were  sorted 
according  to  shell  length  (SL).  in  1-mm  increments  (±0.5 
mm),  ranging  from  5  to  12  mm.  For  each  observation, 
50  snails  of  one  size  class  and  species  were  placed  in  the 
aquarium  in  2  1  of  previously  aerated  tap  water.  One  adult 
P.  simulans  [carapace  length  (CL)  =  28-36  mm]  was  then 
added  to  the  aquarium.  Two  variables  were  recorded  dur- 
ing the  observation  period:  (a)  handling  times  (time  spent 
consuming  a  prey),  and  (b)  ingestion  probabilities  (if  a 
snail  was  eaten,  rejected,  or  had  escaped  from  the  predator 
once  captured).  The  crayfish  (n  =  6)  were  tested  with  all 
size  classes  of  both  species,  randomly,  during  15-min  ob- 
servation times,  over  a  2-week  period.  Crayfish  were  ob- 
served feeding  on  one  size  class  of  one  snail  species  in  all 
observation  periods.  Handling  time  was  defined  as  the 
period  including  the  capture  of  the  snail,  the  consumption 
of  the  snail,  the  crayfish  cleaning  its  mouthparts,  and  the 
movement  forward  by  the  crayfish  to  continue  foraging. 
Each  snail  capture  was  noted,  as  well  as  the  number  of 
snails  that  were  either  consumed  or  rejected.  The  ratio  oi 
number  of  snails  eaten  to  the  number  of  snails  captured 
was  defined  as  the  ingestion  probability. 

Experiment  2:  size-mediated  predator  avoidance 

To  examine  the  relationship  between  snail  size,  pre- 
dation  vulnerability,  and  avoidance  behavior  in  both  snail 
species,  P.  virgata  and  P.  trivolvis  were  sorted  into  five 
size  categories  (4.1-6.0,  6.1-8.0,  8.1-10.0.  10.1-12.0,  and 
12.1-16.0  mm  SL).  A  total  of  100  snails  of  one  species 
was  added  to  each  40-1  aquarium  (25  X  50  X  30  cm)  with 
5  1  of  previously  aerated  tap  water.  Due  to  unequal  num- 
bers available  from  the  field  in  each  size  class,  the  size 
class  categories  contained  unequal  numbers  of  snails.  With 
P.  virgata,  the  numbers  of  snails  per  size  class  added  were: 
10,  30,  30,  25,  and  5  snails  in  each  of  the  increasing  size 
classes,  respectively.  With  P.  trivolvis.  the  numbers  of 
snails  per  size  class  were:  30.  30,  20,  10,  and  10  snails  in 
each  of  the  increasing  size  classes,  respectively. 

To  half  of  the  eight  replicates  per  snail  species,  one 
adult  (CL  =  30-40  mm)  P.  simulans  was  added  at  2200 
h.  The  other  four  replicates  served  as  predator- free  con- 
trols. The  crayfish  were  allowed  to  feed  without  interrup- 
tion for  2  h  in  total  darkness,  then  the  number  of  snails 
out  above  the  waterline.  as  well  as  the  number  of  snails 
eaten,  were  determined  for  each  size  class  and  species. 
Because  all  five  snail  class  sizes  were  included  in  each 
aquarium,  a  split-plot  ANOVA  examined  the  effects  of 
the  two  independent  variables  (presence  or  absence  of 
cravfish  and  snail  size)  on  the  number  of  snails  killed  in 


PREDATOR  AVOIDANCE   IN  FRESHWATER  SNAILS 


389 


each  size  class  (dependent  variable).  A  second  ANOVA 
separately  analyzed  differences  in  the  number  of  surviving 
snails  in  each  size  class  found  above  the  waterline  as  the 
dependent  variable.  Each  snail  species  was  analyzed  sep- 
arately. Because  the  data  were  expressed  as  proportions 
(proportion  of  the  snails  killed  and  the  proportion  of  the 
surviving  snails  above  waterline),  the  data  were  arc-sine 
transformed  prior  to  analysis  (Sokal  and  Rohlf,  1981 ). 

Results 

Experiment  I:  differential  vulnerability  of 
Physella  and  Planorbella 

Handling  times  increased  exponentially  with  increasing 
snail  size  for  both  species  (Fig.  2A).  For  P.  trivolvis,  han- 
dling times  increased  more  rapidly  with  increasing  shell 
size  than  did  the  handling  times  for  P.  virgata.  For  each 
snail  prey,  an  exponential  equation  was  fitted  by  least 
squares  non-linear  regression  to  the  handling  time  data 
of  each  snail  species.  The  resultant  best-fit  non-linear 
regression  between  shell  length  (SL)  and  handling  times 
(HT)  for  P.  virgata  was  HT  =  0.095  e°28(SLl  (n  =  279,  r 
=  0.75).  and  for  P.  trivolvis:  HT  =  0. 1 1 8  e"4:(SLI  (n  =  113, 
r  =  0.74).  For  both  species,  the  best-fit  exponential  equa- 
tions fit  the  data  well,  explaining  74-75%  of  the  observed 
variance  in  the  samples. 

The  ingestion  probabilities  decreased  more  rapidly  with 
increasing  shell  size  for  P.  trivolvis  than  for  P.  virgata  (Fig. 
2).  Approximately  60%  of  the  smallest  P.  trivolvis  (5-7 
mm  SL)  were  not  eaten  once  captured,  and  few  of  the 
larger  P.  trivolvis  (>8  mm  SL)  were  picked  up  by  the 
crayfish.  In  contrast,  all  small  P.  virgata  (<8  mm  SL) 
were  eaten,  once  captured.  The  difference  in  vulnerability 
between  the  two  snail  species  was  significant;  P.  virgata 
were  more  likely  to  be  eaten,  once  captured,  at  all  size 
classes  (Wilcoxin  signed-ranks  test,  T  :=  0,  n  =  5,  P 

<  0.05,  Siegel,  1956).  The  snail  size  at  which  50%  of  prey 
captured  were  rejected  (called  R50)  was  calculated  from 
linear  regression  analyses  run  for  each  individual  crayfish, 
using  the  rejection  data  (log  10  transformed).  The  mean 
R50  for  P.  trivolvis  was  6.5  mm,  and  the  mean  R50  for  P. 
virgata  was  10.7  mm.  The  R5"  for  P.  trivolvis  was  larger 
than  the  R50  for  P.  virgata  for  each  crayfish  used  in  the 
experiment  (Wilcoxin  signed-ranks  test,  T  =  0,  n  =  6,  P 

<  0.05). 

For  both  prey  species,  handling  times  decreased  at  the 
largest  size  class  tested.  The  apparent  decrease  occurred 
because  the  three  smaller  crayfish  used  in  the  study  could 
not  consume  snails  greater  than  8  mm  SL  (in  P.  trivolvis) 
and  12  mm  SL  (in  P.  virgata). 

Experiment  2:  size-mediated  predator  avoidance 

Two-way  analysis  of  variance  results  demonstrated  that 
in  P.  virgata,  only  the  presence  of  a  predator  had  a  sig- 


7         8         9        10 
Snoil    Size  (mm) 


12 


10CM 


7         8         9        10 
Snail    Size  (mm) 


12 


Figure  2.  The  influence  of  snail  size  (shell  length)  on  handling  times 
(A)  and  ingestion  probabilities  (B)  in  Physella  virguta  and  Planorhella 
trivolvia,  fed  upon  by  Procambarus  xiiniilaii.i.  The  error  bars  are  standard 
errors  of  the  mean. 


nificant  effect  on  both  dependent  variables  (the  numbers 
of  surviving  snails  above  the  waterline  and  the  number 
of  snails  killed)  (Fig.  3A.  B,  Table  I).  No  significant  effect 
of  snail  size  was  observed  in  P.  virgata:  all  sizes  of  P. 
virgata  were  equally  vulnerable  and  were  equally  likely 
to  crawl  above  the  waterline.  In  contrast,  for  P.  trivolvis, 
both  independent  variables  (predator  presence,  snail  size) 
and  the  interaction  between  predator  presence  and  snail 
size  all  were  very  significant  (Fig.  4A,  B,  Table  I).  The 
significant  size  effect  was  due  to  the  inverse  relationship 
between  size  and  both  snail  mortality  and  the  number  of 
surviving  snails  above  the  waterline.  Smaller  P.  trivolvis 
were  more  likely  than  larger  individuals  to  be  eaten.  In 
addition  to  being  more  vulnerable  to  P.  simulans  pre- 
dation,  small  (4-6  mm  SL)  P.  trivolvis  displayed  the  most 
prominent  crawlout  response,  with  most  of  the  surviving 
snails  above  the  waterline.  Medium-sized  specimens  (6- 
12  mm  SL)  of  P.  trivolvis  were  intermediate  in  vulnera- 
bility and  were  less  likely  than  smaller  animals  to  display 
the  crawlout  response.  Larger  (12-16  mm  SL)  specimens 
of  P.  trivolvis  were  least  vulnerable  and  did  not  display 
an  increase  in  crawlout  response  over  that  seen  in  pred- 
ator-free control  aquaria.  No  significant  level  of  mortality 


390 


J.  E.  ALEXANDER,  JR.  AND  A.  P.  COVICH 


Physello 


o 

<u 
Q 


c. 

0) 


0> 

CL 


5O 
40 
30 
20 
10 
0 

100 
?l  80 

<T!  5  60 

CD 

o  § 

£  .Q  40 

o  ^2 

£  '5   20 
(/} 


•    Predator 
O    No  Predator 


=O= 


4-6         6-8         8-10      10-12       12-16 
Snail  Size  (mm) 

Figure  3.  Size-mediated  predator  avoidance  and  death  in  Phyxella 
virgata.  The  upper  figure  (A)  is  the  percentage  of  dead  snails  in  each  size 
class  (shell  length),  the  lower  figure  (B)  represents  the  percentage  of  sur- 
viving snails  in  each  size  class  above  the  waterline,  in  both  predator 
(darkened  circles)  and  predator-free  control  (open  circles)  treatments  (n 
=  4  in  both  treatments).  The  error  bars  are  standard  errors  of  the  mean. 


or  crawlout  was  observed  in  the  predator-free  control 
aquaria  in  either  species. 

Of  the  315  surviving  P.  trivolvis,  23  (7.3%)  had  some 
shell  damage  due  to  crayfish.  The  damaged  shells  were 
not  randomly  distributed  among  the  size  classes.  In  the 
two  largest  size  classes,  8.3%  and  5.1%  of  the  surviving 
10-12  mm  and  12-16  mm  SL  size  classes  were  damaged, 
respectively.  The  two  smallest  (4-6  mm  and  6-8  mm  SL) 
size  classes  had  fewer  damaged  shells  than  expected  (1.3% 
and  6.3%,  respectively),  based  on  the  number  of  snails 
originally  available  in  each  size  class,  while  the  interme- 
diate (8-10  mm  SL)  sized  class  had  more  damaged  shells 
than  expected,  15.7%  (X2  goodness-of-fit  test,  X2  =  10.9, 
d.f.  =  4,  P  <  0.05).  In  marked  contrast,  only  one  out  of 
the  314  surviving  P.  virgala  (in  the  8-10  mm  SL  size 
class)  showed  shell  damage  due  to  crayfish  manipulation. 

There  was  no  difference  in  the  predation  intensity  in 
aquaria  housing  P.  trivolvis  or  P.  virgata;  the  crayfish  con- 
sumed equal  numbers  of  P.  virgata  (86)  and  P.  trivolvis 


Table  I 

The  influence  <>l  snail  size  on  miwloiii  hehavtur  in  Physella  virgata 
mill  Planorbella  tnvolvis 


Variable:  %  dead 


Variable:  %  crawlout 


P.  trivolvix      P.  virgata      P  trirohis     P.  virgala 
Factor  (d.f.)  F  F  F  F 


Predator  presence 

(1.6) 

119.3"* 

18.8** 

160.5*** 

146.5*** 

Snail  size 

(4,  24) 

3.0* 

0.4 

15.5*** 

0.3 

Predator  X  Size 

(4,24) 

3.8* 

1.0 

12.7*** 

0.7 

The  table  describes  the  summary  of  the  ANOVA  analyses.  Each  of 
the  dependent  variables  (percent  dead,  percent  surviving  snails  above 
the  waterline)  were  analyzed  separately,  for  each  species.  (Significance 
levels  are  as  follows:  *P  <  0.05:  **/>  <  0.01;  and  *"P  <  0.001.) 


(85)  among  the  four  replicates  in  each  treatment,  sug- 
gesting that  there  was  no  difference  in  hunger  motivation 
:-  the  predators  used.  On  average,  each  crayfish  consumed 


in 


Plonorbello 


•    Predator 
O    No  Predator 


o 

0) 


o 

if 
a> 
Q- 


100 


4-6         6-8         8-10      10-12      12-16 


Snail  Size  (mm) 

Figure  4.     Size-mediated  predator  avoidance  and  death  in  Planorbella 
trivolvis.  The  treatments  and  symbols  are  the  same  as  in  Figure  3. 


PREDATOR  AVOIDANCE  IN  FRESHWATER  SNAILS 


391 


slightly  more  than  21  snails  of  either  prey  species  during 
the  2-h  observation  period. 

Discussion 

In  marine  systems,  gastropod  anti-predator  structures 
and  behaviors  are  common.  Marine  snails  rely  either  on 
strong  shell  architecture  (Palmer,  1979;  Bertness  et  ai. 
1981;  Schmitt,  1982;  Blundon  and  Vermeij,  1983;  Lowell, 
1986)  or  on  escape  and  avoidance  behavior.  Many  marine 
snails  crawl  towards  or  above  the  waterline  to  temporarily 
escape  from  or  avoid  their  predators,  such  as  crabs,  sea 
stars,  and  predatory  gastropods  (Feder,  1963;Ansell,  1969; 
Phillips,  1976;  Vaughn  and  Fisher,  1988).  Like  many  ma- 
rine gastropod  species,  the  crawlout  responses  in  P.  virgata 
and  P.  trivolvis  represent  the  active  use  of  a  potential  refuge 
(the  terrestrial  environment)  that  temporarily  protects 
these  freshwater  snails  from  crayfish  predation. 

In  this  study,  handling  times  (time  spent  consuming 
prey)  and  the  ingestion  probabilities  (probability  of  con- 
suming a  prey)  were  expected  to  differ  between  the  two 
prey,  because  differences  in  vulnerability  existed  due  to 
differences  in  relative  shell  thickness  and  shell  shape  be- 
tween the  two  snail  species.  From  these  results,  it  was 
clear  that  P.  virgata  were  much  more  vulnerable  to  cray- 
fish than  similar-sized  P.  trivolvis.  Crayfish  could  not  con- 
sume large  P.  trivolvis.  because  they  could  not  either  crush 
the  thicker  planispiral  shell  or  manipulate  the  shell  to  a 
position  where  the  mouthparts  could  crush  it  or  chip  the 
thickened  aperture  lip  (pers.  obs.).  Crayfish  often  dropped 
large  P.  trivolvis  (SL  >  6  mm)  after  lengthy  handling  pe- 
riods, and  subsequently  ignored  large  P.  trivolvis  after 
several  unsuccessful  predation  attempts.  In  contrast,  in 
all  size  classes,  the  thinner,  elongated  spiral  shell  of  P. 
virgata  could  be  manipulated  and  crushed  by  the  same 
crayfish,  strongly  suggesting  that  specimens  of  P.  virgata 
were  more  vulnerable  to  crayfish  predation  than  P.  tri- 
volvis. Crayfish  either  crushed  the  shell  at  the  body  whorl, 
chipped  away  at  the  aperture  lip,  or  had  broken  off  the 
shell  spire  (pers.  obs.).  Because  their  shells  provided  little 
structural  defense,  specimens  of  all  size  classes  of  P.  virgata 
were  equally  vulnerable  to  crayfish  predation  and  thus 
were  equally  likely  to  crawl  above  the  waterline  (Fig.  3). 
In  examining  the  surviving  snails  from  the  second  ex- 
periment, only  one  living  specimen  of  P.  virgata  with  a 
damaged  shell  was  observed  in  the  experiment,  strong 
indirect  evidence  that,  once  a  specimen  of  P.  virgata  was 
captured,  the  snail  was  usually  eaten.  In  addition,  when 
foraging  on  P.  virgata,  crayfish  almost  always  were  able 
to  effectively  handle  and  consume  P.  virgata  encountered, 
as  shown  by  the  high  ingestion  probabilities  (Fig.  2).  For 
P.  trivolvis,  23  (7.3%)  of  the  surviving  animals  recovered 
in  the  second  experiment  were  observed  with  some  dam- 


age, suggesting  that  they  survived  a  predatory  encounter 
with  the  crayfish. 

At  any  given  size,  specimens  of  P.  trivolvis  required  2- 
4  times  the  handling  time  by  crayfish  for  successful  pre- 
dation than  did  similar-sized  P.  virgata  (Fig.  2A),  indi- 
cating the  much  greater  difficulty  in  crushing  the  P.  tri- 
volvis shells.  Stein  et  al.  (1984),  comparing  the  prey  value 
of  a  physid  (Physa  sp.)  and  a  planorbid  (Helisoitui  sp.)  to 
redear  sunfish  (Lcponiis  microlophus),  noted  that  redear 
sunfish  weakly  selected  the  physid  over  the  planorbid,  but 
that  size  selection  did  not  occur  within  either  genus.  The 
force  required  to  crush  the  physid  (3  Newtons  for  10  mm 
SL  Physa  lying  aperture  down)  was  less  than  that  required 
to  crush  the  planorbid  (4  Newtons  for  10  mm  SL  Heli- 
soma  lying  on  its  side),  but  the  difference  in  force  was  not 
dramatically  different.  The  shells  were  crushed  in  this  way 
because  sunfish  were  observed  orienting  snails  between 
their  pharyngeal  gill  plates  (their  crushing  surfaces)  so  as 
to  crush  the  minimal  dimension  of  the  shell.  Crayfish  in 
our  study  crushed  snail  shells  primarily  by  chipping  with 
their  mandibles  at  the  shell  aperture,  holding  the  snail 
with  their  maxillipeds.  Sometimes,  crayfish  appeared  to 
use  their  chelae  to  balance  and  press  the  shell  against  their 
mandibles.  Although  we  did  not  measure  the  force  re- 
quired for  crayfish  to  crush  P.  virgata  and  P.  trivolvis  shells, 
nor  did  we  measure  the  shell  thicknesses,  the  data  suggest 
that  crayfish  could  more  easily  crush  P.  virgata  shells  than 
P.  trivolvis  shells,  because  crayfish  primarily  attempt  to 
break  the  aperture  lip,  particularly  in  P.  trivolvis.  and  not 
the  entire  shell. 

Antipredator  mechanisms  may  be  quite  dissimilar  in 
closely  related  gastropod  species.  Two  congeneric  species 
of  marine  snails,  Tegula  eiseni  and  T.  aureotincta,  differ 
in  their  predator  defenses;  T.  aureotincta  performed 
avoidance  behaviors  to  gastropod  and  asteroid  predators, 
while  T.  eiseni  depended  more  on  shell  morphology  for 
defense  (Schmitt,  1982).  Physella  virgata  relies  on  behav- 
ioral avoidance  much  more  exclusively  than  does  P.  tri- 
volvis, which  appears  to  rely  more  on  predator  avoidance 
when  young,  and  on  shell  strength  as  larger  adults. 

The  correspondence  between  the  reactivity  of  snails  of 
a  given  size  class  and  their  vulnerability  was  expected,  if 
predator  avoidance  behavior  (crawlout)  has  some  costs 
associated  with  reacting  inappropriately  to  the  potential 
threat  of  predation.  Crawling  to  or  above  the  waterline 
could  expose  the  snail  to  other  predators,  including  birds, 
and  certain  insects,  such  as  belostomatids  (Crowl  and  Al- 
exander, 1989;  Kesler  and  Munns,  1990).  Further  costs 
to  crawlout  behavior  include  decreased  foraging  time  (if 
the  animals  cannot  forage  on  food  above  the  waterline), 
decreased  opportunities  for  reproduction,  and  desiccation 
(Alexander  and  Covich,  1991). 


392 


J.  E.  ALEXANDER.  JR.  AND  A.  P.  COVICH 


The  differences  in  the  antipredator  responses  between 
the  two  species  may  be  influenced  by  differences  in  selec- 
tive pressures  caused  by  the  distinct  physiological  adap- 
tations used  by  the  two  snails  in  their  respective  micro- 
habitats  (McMahon,  1983).  Planorbella  trivolvis.  with  its 
well-developed  neomorphic  gill,  and  its  more  efficient  re- 
spiratory pigment  (hemoglobin),  is  much  more  aquatic 
than  P.  virgata,  which  retains  an  air-filled  mantle  cavity 
(lung)  as  the  major  organ  of  gas  exchange.  Physella  virgata 
makes  periodic  excursions  to  the  surface  to  renew  its  ox- 
ygen store,  and  subsequently  is  limited  to  shallow  water 
near-shore  habitats  or  those  habitats  with  structure  (i.e., 
aquatic  macrophytes  or  woody  debris)  extending  above 
the  waterline.  Planorbella  trivolvis.  with  a  much  greater 
capacity  for  aquatic  gas  exchange,  makes  excursions  into 
much  deeper  water,  where  crawlout  sites  are  likely  to  be 
unavailable.  In  P.  virgata,  physiologically  restricted  to 
shallow,  near-shore  waters,  selective  pressures  may  have 
caused  a  retention  of  a  strong  crawlout  response  to  avoid 
predators  and  reduced  pressure  for  the  development  of  a 
structurally  predator-resistant  shell.  In  contrast,  P.  trivol- 
vis, whose  range  (particularly  in  adults)  extends  into 
deeper  water  and  consequently  has  little  access  to  terres- 
trial refugia,  selection  pressures  may  have  been  towards 
development  of  a  structurally  predator-resistant  shell  and 
a  reduced  dependence  on  a  crawlout  response. 

Because  they  can  be  the  dominant  primary  consumers 
in  some  habitats,  mollusks  and  decapod  crustaceans  play 
important  roles  in  many  aquatic  communities.  Many  are 
herbivorous,  detritivorous,  or  omnivorous,  and  are  im- 
portant for  cycling  nutrients  and  providing  energy  in  the 
form  of  variously  sized  food  items  for  higher  trophic  level 
consumers  (Ansell,  1969;  Momot  el  ai,  1978;  Grimm, 
1988).  The  study  of  the  behavioral  interplay  between 
freshwater  snails  and  crayfish  is  essential  in  understanding 
how  these  behavioral  processes  influence  predator-prey 
dynamics  and  community  composition.  Rapid  snail  es- 
cape and  avoidance  behavior,  and  the  subsequent  decrease 
in  encounter  probabilities,  suggest  that  in  some  structur- 
ally complex  habitats,  such  as  macrophyte-dominated  lit- 
toral zones  or  forested  wetland  areas,  vertical  migration 
above  the  waterline  is  an  adaptive  response  to  crayfish 
predation. 

Acknowledgments 

The  first  author  thanks  his  Ph.D.  advisory  committee: 
R.  Mellgren,  D.  Mock,  F.  Sonleitner,  and  T.  Yoshino,  for 
their  suggestions.  Various  versions  of  the  manuscript  and 
statistical  analyses  were  improved  by  the  efforts  of  T. 
Crowl,  K.  Brown,  and  P.  Rutledge.  We  thank  R.  Mc- 
Mahon for  his  help  with  snail  identification,  figure  draw- 
ings, for  providing  insight  into  the  different  physiological 


constraints  on  the  two  snails,  and  for  critically  evaluating 
the  final  manuscript.  Two  anonymous  reviewers  helped 
to  evaluate  the  final  manuscript.  The  research  was  sup- 
ported by  research  and  teaching  assistantships  from  the 
Department  of  Zoology,  University  of  Oklahoma,  and  by 
NSF  grant  BSR  8500773. 

Literature  Cited 

Alexander,  J.  E.,  Jr.  1987.     Predator-prey  interactions  between  fresh- 
water snails  and  crayfish.  Ph.D.  dissertation.  University  of  Oklahoma. 
Alexander,  J.  E.,  Jr.,  and  A.  P.  Covich.  1991.     Predator  avoidance  in 

the  freshwater  snail  Physella  virgala  to  the  crayfish  Procambarus 

simulant.  Oecologia  (in  press). 
Ansell,  A.  D.  1969.     Defensive  adaptations  to  predation  in  the  mollusca. 

Proc.  Mar.  Biol.  Assoc.  India  Symp.  Ser.  3:  487-512. 
Bertness,  M.  D.,  S.  D.  Garrity,  and  S.  C.  Levings.  1981.     Predation 

pressure  and  gastropod  foraging:  a  tropical-temperate  comparison. 

Evolution  35:  995-1007. 
Blundon,  J.  A.,  and  G.  J.  Vermeij.  1983.     Effect  of  shell  repair  on  shell 

strength  in  the  gastropod  Liltorina  irrorata.  Mar  Bio/.  76:  41-45. 
Brown,  K.  M.,  and  D.  R.  DeYries.  1985.     Predation  and  the  distribution 

and  abundance  of  a  pulmonate  pond  snail.  Oecologia  66:  93-99. 
Brown,  K.  M.,  and  B.  H.  Strouse.  1988.     Relative  vulnerability  of  six 

freshwater  gastropods  to  the  leech  Nephelopxix  obscura  (Verrill). 

Freshwater  Bin!.  19:  157-165. 
Covich,  A.  P.  1981.     Chemical  refugia  from  predation  for  thin-shelled 

gastropods  in  a  sulfide-ennched  stream.    Yerh.   Int.    1'ereinigitng 

Theorel.  Augewandte  Limnol.  21:  1632-1636. 
Crowl,  T.  A.  1990.     Life-history  strategies  of  a  freshwater  snail  in  response 

to  stream  permanence  and  predation:  balancing  conflicting  demands. 

Oecologia  84:  238-243. 
Crowl,  T.  A.,  and  J.  E.  Alexander,  Jr.  1989.     Parental  care  and  foraging 

ability  in  male  waterbugs  (Belostoma  flumineum).  Can  J  Zoo/.  67: 

513-515. 
Crowl,  T.  A.,  and  A.  P.  Covich.  1990.     Predator-induced  life-history 

shifts  in  a  freshwater  snail.  Science  247:  949-95 1 . 
Feder,  H.  M.  1963.     Gastropod  defensive  responses  and  their  effective- 
ness in  reducing  predation  by  starfishes.  Ecology  44:  505-512. 
Grimm,  N.  B.  1988.     Role  of  macroinvertebrates  in  nitrogen  dynamics 

of  a  desert  stream.  Ecology  69:  1884-1893. 
Hanson,  J.  M.,  P.  A.  Chambers,  and  E.  E.  Prepas.  1990.     Selective 

foraging  by  the  crayfish  Orconectes  virilis  and  its  impact  on  macro- 
invertebrates,  freshwater  Biol.  24:  69-80. 
Kesler,  D.  K.,  and  \V.  R.  Munns,  Jr.  1990.     Predation  by  Belostoma 

flumineum  (Hemiptera):  an  important  cause  of  mortality  in  freshwater 

snails.  J.  N.  Am.  Ben/hoi.  Soc.  8:  342-350. 
Lodge,  D.  M.,  K.  M.  Brown,  S.  P.  Klosiewski,  R.  A.  Stein,  A.  P.  Covich, 

B.  K.  Leathers,  and  C.  Bronmark.  1987.     Distribution  of  freshwater 

snails:  spatial  scale  and  the  relative  importance  of  physicochemical 

and  biotic  factors.  Am.  Malacol.  Union  5:  73-84. 
Lowell,  R.  B.  1986.     Crab  predation  on  limpets:  predator  behavior  and 

defensive  features  of  the  shell  morphology  of  the  prey.  Biol  Bull 

171:  577-596. 
McMahon,  R.  F.  1983.     Physiological  ecology  of  freshwater  pulmonates. 

Pp.  359-430  in  The  Mollusca.  Vol.  6,  Ecology.  W.  D.  Russell-Hunter, 

ed.  Academic  Press,  Inc..  Orlando.  FL. 
Momot,  \V.  T.,  H.  Gowing,  and  P.  D.  Jones.  1978.     The  dynamics  of 

crayfish  and  their  role  in  ecosystems.  Am.  Midi.  Nat.  99:  10-35. 
Palmer,  A.  R.  1979.     Fish  predation  and  the  evolution  of  gastropod 

shell  sculpture:  experimental  and  geographic  evidence.  Evolution  33: 

697-7 1 3. 


PREDATOR  AVOIDANCE   IN  FRESHWATER  SNAILS 


393 


Peckarsky,  B.  L.  1984.  Predator-prey  interactions  among  aquatic  in- 
sects. Pp.  l%-254  in  The  Ecology  of  Aqital ic  //;vr/v.  V.  H.  Resh 
and  D.  M.  Rosenberg,  eds.  Praeger  Scientific,  New  York. 

Phillips,  D.  \V.  1976.  The  effect  of  a  species-specific  avoidance  response 
to  predatory  starfish  on  the  intertidal  distribution  of  two  gastropods. 
Oeco/ogia  23:  83-94. 

Schmitt.  R.  J.  1982.  Consequences  of  dissimilar  defenses  against  pre- 
dation  in  a  subtidal  marine  community.  Ecology  63:  1588-1601. 

Siegel,  S.  1956.  \onparametric  Statistics  For  Tilt  Behavioral  Science* 
McGraw-Hill.  Inc..  New  York. 

Sih,  A.  1982.  Foraging  strategies  and  the  avoidance  of  predation  by 
an  aquatic  insect,  h'olonecta  hoffmanni.  Ecology  63:  786-796. 

Sih,  A.  198-4.  The  behavioral  response  race  between  predator  and  prey. 
Am.  Nat  123:  143-150. 

Sih,  A.  1986.  Antipredator  responses  and  the  perception  of  danger  by 
mosquito  larvae.  Ecology  67:  434-441. 

Snydcr,  N.  F.  R.  1967.  An  alarm  reaction  of  aquatic  gastropods  to 
intraspecific  extract.  Cornell  University  Agricultural  Experimental 
Station  Memoir  403. 

Sokal,  R.  R.,  and  F.  J.  Rohlf.  1981.  Biometry.  2nd  ed.  W.  W.  Freeman 
and  Co..  San  Francisco. 


Stein.  R.  A.  1977.  Selective  predation,  optimal  foraging,  and  the  pred- 
ator-prey interaction  between  fish  and  crayfish.  Ecology  58:  1237- 
1253. 

Stein,  R.  A.,  C.  G.  Goodman,  and  F,.  A.  Marschall.  1984.  Using  time 
and  energetic  measures  of  cost  in  estimating  prey  value  for  fish  pred- 
ators. Ecology  65:  702-715. 

Townsend.  C.  R..  and  T.  K.  McCarthy.  1980.  On  the  defence  strategy 
of  Physa  lonlinalis  (L.).  a  freshwater  pulmonate  snail.  Oecologia  46: 
75-79. 

Vaughn,  C.  C..  and  F.  M.  Fisher.  1988.  Vertical  migration  as  a  refuge 
from  predation  in  intertidal  marsh  snails:  a  field  test.  /  E\p.  Mar. 
Biol  Ecol.  123:  163-176. 

Vermeij,  G.  J.  I982a.  Phenotypic  evolution  in  a  poorly  dispersing  snail 
after  arrival  of  a  predator.  Nature  299:  349-350. 

Vermeij,  G.  J.  1982b.  Unsuccessful  predation  and  evolution.  Am.  Nat. 
120:  701-720. 

Vermeij,  G.  J.,  and  A.  P.  Covich.  1978.  Coevolution  of  freshwater 
gastropods  and  their  predators.  Am.  Nat.  112:  833-843. 

Werner,  E.  E.,  and  D.  J.  Hall.  1988.  Ontogenetic  habitat  shifts  in  blue- 
gill:  the  foraging  rate-predation  risk  trade-off.  Ecology  69:  1352-1366. 


Reference:  Rial  Hull  180:  394-405.  (June.  1991) 


Shape  Variation  in  Hydractiniid  Hydroids 

NEIL  W.  BLACKSTONE1  AND  LEO  W.  BUSS1 2 

Department  of  Biology,  and  ^Department  of  Geology  and  Geophysics, 
Yale  Universitv,  New  Haven,  Connecticut  065 II 


Abstract.  Colonies  of  hydractiniid  hydroids  consist  of 
feeding  polyps  connected  by  a  common  gastrovascular 
system.  The  gastrovascular  system  consists  of  stolons, 
which  enclose  gastrovascular  canals.  Stolons  may  be  fused 
into  a  stolonal  mat  or  extend  from  the  periphery  of  the 
colony.  Hydractinia  forms  a  stolonal  mat  early  in  colony 
development;  Podocoryne.  on  the  other  hand,  does  not. 
To  facilitate  comparisons  of  these  taxa.  we  propose  a  sim- 
ple shape  metric,  perimeter/^ area,  and  show  that  this 
measure:  ( 1 )  correlates  closely  with  relative  amounts  of 
peripheral  stolon  and  stolonal  mat  structures  in  Hydrac- 
tinia, (2)  permits  analyses  of  within-  and  between-species 
variation  of  growth  morphology  in  Podocoryne  and  Hy- 
dractinia, and  (3)  allows  quantitative  analysis  of  breeding 
studies  of  Hydractinia,  both  before  and  after  stolonal  mat 
formation  in  the  progeny. 

Introduction 

Hydractiniid  hydroids  encrust  hard  substrata  in  the  sea. 
Hydractinia  echinata  and  related  species  are  commonly 
found  on  the  shells  of  hermit  crabs  and  often  exhibit  a 
species-specific  correlation  with  host  hermit  crabs  (Buss 
and  Yund,  1989;  Cunningham  et  al,  in  press).  Podocoryne 
carnea  also  encrust  hermit  crab  shells,  but  commonly  in- 
habit other  substrata  as  well  (Edwards,  1972;  Mills,  1976). 
Colony  development  in  both  taxa  begins  with  the  meta- 
morphosis of  the  planula  larvae  into  a  primary  polyp. 
Runner-like  stolons  extend  from  the  primary  polyp.  Sto- 
lons encase  fluid-filled,  gastrovascular  canals  that  are  con- 
tinuous with  the  gastrovascular  cavity  of  the  polyp.  Po- 
docoryne continues  to  develop  in  this  way,  i.e.,  by  lineal 
extension  of  the  stolons,  initiation  of  new  stolon  tips,  and 
iteration  of  feeding  polyps  on  the  stolons  (Braverman, 
1963;  McFadden,  1986).  Stolons  in  Hydractinia,  however. 

Received  7  August  1990:  accepted  19  February  1991. 


quickly  fuse  to  form  a  continuous  stolonal  mat,  which 
shows  sheet-like  growth,  and  from  which  extend  varying 
amounts  of  peripheral  stolons  (McFadden  et  al.,  1984; 
Blackstone  and  Yund,  1989;  Buss  and  Grosberg,  1990). 
Figure  1  provides  rough  schemata  of  the  differences  in 
form  between  these  taxa. 

While  morphological  variation  within  each  taxon  has 
been  compared  and  related  to  ecological  characteristics 
(e.g.,  competitive  ability;  see  McFadden  et  al..  1984; 
McFadden,  1986;  Yund,  1987;  Buss  and  Grosberg,  1990), 
quantitative  comparisons  of  between-taxa  variation  have 
been  hampered  by  the  differences  in  growth  form,  i.e., 
the  presence  of  a  stolonal  mat  in  Hydractinia  and  its  ab- 
sence in  Podocoryne.  For  instance,  competitive  ability 
among  strains  of  Hydractinia  has  been  shown  to  correlate 
with  relative  amounts  of  peripheral  stolon  and  stolonal 
mat  structures  (measured  using  several  methods,  see 
McFadden  et  al..  1984;  Yund,  1987;  Buss  and  Grosberg, 
1990),  but  such  measures  cannot  be  applied  to  Podoco- 
ryne. 

To  facilitate  comparisons  of  biological  traits  between 
Podocoryne  and  Hydractinia.  we  propose  a  simple  mea- 
sure of  morphology  that  can  be  used  in  both  taxa.  We 
show  that  this  measure  correlates  with  ratios  of  peripheral 
stolon  and  stolonal  mat  structures  in  Hydractinia.  and 
we  use  this  measure  to  examine  morphological  variation 
and  its  genetic  basis  both  between  and  within  Podocoryne 
and  Hydractinia.  Finally,  we  relate  this  variation  to  eco- 
logical, evolutionary,  and  developmental  aspects  of  these 
species  and  discuss  the  relevance  to  other  clonal  taxa  as 
well. 

Materials  and  Methods 

Growth  morphology  and  shape 

We  suggest  treating  hydractiniid  hydroids  as  geometric 
shapes  for  purposes  of  comparison.  We  prefer  Bookstein's 


394 


HYDRACTIN1ID  HYDROID  SHAPE 


395 


(1978:  p.  8)  definition:  "...  a  shape  is  an  outline-with- 
landmarks  from  which  all  information  about  position, 
scale,  and  orientation  has  been  drained,"  with  the  qual- 
ification that  hydroid  colonies  have  no  reliable  morpho- 
logical landmarks.  Further,  the  aspects  of  hydractiniid 
growth  morphology  of  particular  interest  are  essentially 
two-dimensional,  comprising  those  portions  of  the  colony 
that  adhere  to  the  substratum.  Although  there  are  so- 
phisticated techniques  available  for  the  analysis  of  two- 
dimensional  shapes-without-landmarks  (e.g..  Lohman, 
1983;  Person  et  a/..  1985),  we  will  take  a  simpler  approach. 
The  terms  previously  used  to  categorize  hydractiniid 
growth  morphology  (many  peripheral  stolons  =  "net 
type,"  few  peripheral  stolons  =  "mat  type,"  see  Hauen- 
schild,  1954)  point  out  an  intuitively  obvious  correlation 
between  two-dimensional  growth  morphology  and  shape. 
Colonies  with  few  peripheral  stolons  often  show  approx- 
imately circular  growth  forms,  while  colonies  with  many 
peripheral  stolons  exhibit  more  irregular  shapes  (Fig.  1). 

An  appropriate  "size-free"  metric  to  quantify  these  dif- 
ferences in  shape  is  perimeter f\ area  (cj..  Gould,  1973; 
Patton.  1975).  We  point  out  several  properties  of  this 
measure,  by  way  of  introducing  it  to  morphological  studies 
of  encrusting  clonal  organisms.  First,  regardless  of  scale, 
this  measure  is  constant  for  a  given  geometric  shape.  For 
instance,  this  measure  will  equal  2\v  for  a  circle,  4  for  a 
square,  =4.5  for  an  equilateral  triangle,  =  5.4  for  a  "first- 
aid"  sign,  =  5.7  for  a  cross  (length  of  the  long  arm  is  twice 
that  of  the  three  short  arms),  and  so  on.  In  each  case, 
these  values  are  constant  regardless  of  the  actual  size  of 
the  object  as  long  as  the  same  units  are  used  to  measure 
both  perimeter  and  area.  Second,  while  the  same  geo- 
metric shapes  will  have  similar  peri  meter/]/ area  values, 
shapes  with  the  same  perimeter/} 'area  need  not  be  the 
same.  In  fact,  for  encrusting  clonal  organisms,  no  two 
shapes  are  likely  to  be  the  same,  yet  many  may  have  sim- 
ilar peri  meter/]/ area  values.  This  shape  metric  thus  only 
assesses  the  degree  of  circularity  of  a  shape.  Shapes  with 
values  close  to  2^  approach  perfect  circularity,  while 
highly  non-circular  shapes  have  much  larger  values. 
Third,  perimeter/^ area  has  a  minimum  at  2  VTT,  possible 
values  thus  have  a  lower  bound,  and  their  distributions 
may  be  skewed.  Note  that  this  is  not  unusual;  most  mor- 
phometric  measurements  have  a  lower  bound  at  zero  and 
thus  may  form  skewed  distributions.  Regardless  of  the 
lower  bound  (0  or  2  Vir),  a  log-transformation  usually 
provides  distributions  suitable  for  parametric  analysis  (see 
Sokal  and  Rohlf,  1981). 

The  utility  of  this  measure  may  be  visualized  by  com- 
paring a  plot  of  perimeter  and  area  for  two  sibling  Hy- 
dractinia  colonies  (Fig.  2)  grown  from  primary  polyps 
under  standard  conditions  (see  McFadden  et  at..  1984). 
While  the  perimeter  versus  area  trajectories  fluctuate  as 


stolons  branch  and  fuse,  it  is  clear  that  the  colony  with  a 
greater  amount  of  stolons  projecting  from  the  center  has 
a  larger  perimeter  for  a  given  area  and  larger 
perimeter/]/ area  values.  To  assess  quantitatively  the  ca- 
pacity of  this  shape  metric  to  assay  the  amount  of  pe- 
ripheral stolons,  we  measured  the  correlation  between 
perimeter/]/ area  and  peripheral  stolon  development  for 
the  242  Hvdractinia  colonies  used  in  a  breeding  study 
(see  protocol  below).  At  age  50  days,  each  colony  was 
measured  using  a  digital  image  analysis  system.  Briefly, 
an  Eyecom  II  camera  attached  to  a  Wild  Makroskop  was 
used  to  project  each  colony  onto  a  black-and-white  mon- 
itor (640  X  480  pixels;  note  that  the  pixels  are  orthogonal 
and  that  the  length  of  a  pixel  is  the  same  in  either  direc- 
tion). Points  on  the  video  image  of  each  colony  were  re- 
corded with  a  digitizing  tablet  interfaced  with  a  DEC  PDP- 
1 1  minicomputer.  The  outline  of  each  colony  was  traced 
with  points  at  5  pixel  intervals,  and  the  perimeter  and 
area  were  computed.  The  outline  of  the  stolonal  mat,  i.e., 
the  fused  stolons  (see  Fig.  1),  was  also  traced,  and  the 
perimeter  and  area  were  measured.  Scales  ranged  from 
150  pixels/mm  for  the  smallest  colonies  to  25  pixels/mm 
for  the  largest  colonies.  Over  this  range  of  observation, 
these  colonies  are  not  fractal,  i.e.,  they  do  possess  a  char- 
acteristic scale  and  do  not  show  self-similarity  over  the 
different  scales  of  observation  employed  here  (although 
self-similarity  may  be  apparent  using  other  scales  of  ob- 
servation). Thus,  while  smaller  colonies  were  measured 
with  slightly  greater  resolution,  this  did  not  bias  the  results 
in  a  systematic  fashion.  Data  were  transferred  to  an  IBM- 
PC  and  uploaded  to  an  IBM  3083  mainframe  where  anal- 
ysis was  done  using  SAS  software. 

Comparing  peripheral  stolon  development  to  colony 
shape  entails  methodologic  problems.  Logical  measures 
of  peripheral  stolon  development  involve  a  measure  of 
the  total  size  of  the  colony  divided  by  the  size  of  the  sto- 
lonal mat  (e.g.,  total  colony  perimeter/stolonal  mat  pe- 
rimeter and  total  colony  area/stolonal  mat  area).  Given 
the  nature  of  colony  growth  (i.e.,  peripheral  stolons  pro- 
jecting from  a  central  area  of  stolonal  mat.  Fig.  1 ),  the 
extent  to  which  these  ratios  are  greater  than  1  will  measure 
the  amount  of  peripheral  stolons.  A  straightforward  pro- 
cedure would  be  to  correlate  these  ratios  to  the  total  pe- 
rimeter divided  by  the  square  root  of  the  total  area  (i.e., 
the  shape  metric).  However,  this  could  result  in  autocor- 
relation, because  both  ratios  necessarily  contain  measures 
of  the  overall  size  of  the  colony  (either  total  perimeter  or 
total  area).  We  measured  these  correlations  and  then  as- 
sessed the  effects  of  autocorrelation  by  adjusting  the  cor- 
related ratios  to  remove  similar  variables  from  each.  For 
instance,  the  correlation  of  the  ratio,  colony  perimeter/ 
stolonal  mat  perimeter,  to  the  ratio,  peri  meter/]/ area,  can 


396 


N.  W.  BLACKSTONE  AND  L.  W.  BUSS 


be  considered  equivalent  to  the  correlation  of  stolonal 
mat  perimeter  to  ^larea  if  there  are  no  effects  of  autocor- 
relation. Further,  we  considered  the  biological  meaning 
of  correlations  between  stolonal  mat  size  and  total  colony 
size  and  whether  these  correlations  support  our  interpre- 
tation of  the  shape  metric.  Spearman's  coefficient  of  rank 
correlation  (rv)  was  used;  this  coefficient  is  less  sensitive 
to  the  statistical  pecularities  of  ratios  than  parametric  cor- 
relation coefficients  (see  Sokal  and  Rohlf,  1981),  although 
here  both  coefficients  were  similar  for  all  correlations. 

Shape  variation  in  Podocoryne  and  Hydractinia 

The  perimeter/^  area  measure  was  used  to  compare 
morphological  variation  within  and  between  field-col- 
lected colonies  of  Podocoryne  and  Hydractinia  using  the 
technique  of  clonal  repeatibility,  i.e..  comparing  clonal 
replicates  of  the  same  colony  to  gauge  broad-sense  her- 
itability  (Falconer,  1981).  Colonies  were  collected  from 
an  intertidal  site  near  Guilford,  Connecticut,  where  Po- 
docoryne carnea  and  Hydractinia  symbiolongicarpus,  a 
sibling  species  of  H.  echinaia.  commonly  co-occur  (Buss 
and  Yund,  1989).  although  Podocoryne  is  much  less 
abundant  than  Hydractinia.  When  reproductive  polyps 
are  present,  these  species  can  be  easily  distinguished:  Po- 
docoryne produces  free-swimming  medusae,  while  Hy- 
dractinia lacks  a  medusoid  stage  and  produces  fixed  gon- 
ophores  (Mills,  1976).  Using  a  dissecting  microscope,  Po- 
docoryne colonies  were  identified  from  large  collections 
of  all  hydroid-bearing  hermit  crab  shells.  Relatively  few 
colonies  contained  reproductive  polyps;  hence,  tentative 
identifications  were  made  on  the  basis  of  general  patterns 
of  colony  appearance  (in  this  area,  Podocoryne  has  few 
spines  and  usually  co-occurs  with  algal  epibionts)  and 
feeding  polyp  morphology  (in  this  area,  Podocoryne  tends 
to  have  smaller  polyps,  a  more  pronounced  hypostome, 
and  shorter,  more  tapered  tentacles).  In  this  way,  60  col- 
onies were  tentatively  identified  as  Podocoryne  and  were 
labelled  with  numbered  bee  tags  attached  with  cyanoac- 
rylate  adhesive.  Colonies  were  maintained  in  40-liter 
aquaria  with  undergravel  filters  (20  colonies  per  tank)  at 
16°C.  Colonies  were  fed  3-day-old  brine  shrimp  nauplii 
(also  grown  at  16°C)  every  other  day,  and  25%  of  the 
water  was  changed  twice  a  week.  In  1-2  weeks  all  colonies 
were  reproductive  (tentative  identifications  were  correct 
in  all  cases).  Medusae  from  each  colony  were  isolated  and 
raised  in  finger  bowls  at  16°C.  Each  day.  medusae  were 
examined  under  a  dissecting  microscope,  fed  brine  shrimp, 
and  transferred  to  fresh  seawater.  Medusae  were  raised  to 
sexual  maturity  (7-14  days),  and  the  sex  of  the  parent 
colony  was  determined  by  the  morphology  of  the  gonads 
(Rees,  1941;  Edwards,  1972;  identity  as  P.  carnea  was 
also  verified  by  examining  the  medusae,  see  Edwards, 


1972;  Mills,  1976).  From  the  original  60  colonies.  10  male 
and  5  female  colonies  were  selected  using  a  pseudo-ran- 
dom number  generator.  Sixty  Hydractinia  colonies  were 
haphazardly  collected  from  the  same  site  and  maintained 
in  the  same  fashion.  Compared  to  Podocoryne.  Hydrac- 
tinia  requires  more  time  to  mature  (cf.  Hauenschild,  1956; 
Braverman.  1963),  but  within  two  months  all  colonies 
were  fully  reproductive,  whereupon  they  were  sexed,  la- 
belled, and  10  male  and  5  female  colonies  were  selected 
with  a  pseudo-random  number  generator.  Previous  in- 
vestigations (McFadden  et  a/..  1984;  Buss  and  Grosberg, 
1 990)  have  shown  that  colony  morphology  does  not  differ 
on  the  basis  of  sex;  nevertheless,  equal  numbers  of  each 
sex  from  each  species  were  included  in  this  study. 

For  morphological  comparisons,  colonies  were  surgi- 
cally explanted  onto  22  mm2  glass  cover  slips  and  held 
in  place  with  loops  of  thread  until  attachment  whereupon 
the  threads  were  removed  (see  McFadden  et  al..  1984; 
explants  of  3-5  feeding  polyps  were  used).  Because  of  the 
work  involved,  comparisons  were  made  using  five  field- 
collected  colonies  of  each  species  at  a  time.  Five  explants 
(hereafter  "replicates")  for  each  of  the  five  field-collected 
colonies  (hereafter  "strains")  for  both  Podocoryne  and 
Hydractinia  (hereafter  "species")  were  grown  in  a  floating 
rack  at  16°C.  Three  "racks"  were  used  over  a  two-month 
period;  rack  is  thus  a  proxy  for  time  effects.  Each  rack 
consisted  of  two  side-by-side  rows  of  slots;  cover  slips  were 
arranged  so  that  the  five  replicates  for  each  strain  occupied 
consecutive  slots;  strains  of  each  species  were  randomly 
paired,  alternating  right  and  left  sides.  The  formal  analysis 
thus  consists  of  a  four-level  nested  analysis  of  variance 
(see  Sokal  and  Rohlf,  1981 ).  Replicates  are  nested  within 
strains,  which  are  nested  within  species,  which  are  nested 
within  racks.  Such  an  analysis  accounts  for  all  sources  of 
variation  except  position  within  racks.  Position  effects  can 
be  assessed  by  designating  five  positions  within  each  rack; 
each  position  then  contains  the  replicates  from  a  pair  of 
Podocoryne  and  Hydractinia  strains.  The  analysis  then 
becomes  replicates  within  species  within  positions  within 
racks.  Outcome  variables  were  analyzed  in  both  ways. 

Using  the  above  protocols,  perimeter/} 'area  measures 
were  taken,  and  counts  of  polyps  and  total  area  measures 
were  also  recorded.  Variables  were  measured  at  7  and  14 
days  after  explanting;  specific  growth  rates  (see  Blackstone, 
1987;  Blackstone  and  Yund,  1989)  for  polyp  (polyp/polyp- 
dav)  and  area  (mnr /mnr /-day)  were  also  calculated  for 
this  interval.  Each  rate  was  calculated  by  increment  in 
number  or  area  (for  polyp  number  and  total  area  respec- 
tively) per  time  increment  per  initial  number  or  area. 
While  technically  "specific"  refers  to  "divided  by  mass," 
any  measure  of  size  can  be  used,  provided  the  same  units 
are  used  in  the  numerator  and  denominator,  since  a  spe- 
cific growth  rate  has  units  of  I/time. 


HYDRACTINIID  HYDROID  SHAPE 


397 


Breeding  studies 

While  studies  of  clonal  repeatibility  can  establish  broad- 
sense  heritabilities  for  a  trait,  breeding  studies  can  provide 
further  insight  into  the  nature  of  the  genetic  variation 
underlying  a  trait  (Falconer,  1981).  With  the  same  5  fe- 
male and  10  male  Hydractinia  colonies  used  above,  10 
crosses  (2  males  per  female)  were  designated  using  a 
pseudo-random  number  generator,  and  additional  mating 
experiments  were  done  to  insure  that  all  individuals  be- 
longed to  the  same  species  (see  Buss  and  Yund,  1989). 
Matings  were  carried  out  every  several  days  for  a  month. 
Pairs  of  male  and  female  colonies  were  isolated  in  the 
dark  overnight;  morning  light  triggered  gamete  release  (see 
Yund  el  al,  1987,  and  references  therein).  Embryos  were 
transferred  to  fresh  seawater  and  kept  for  3-4  days  with 
a  daily  water  change.  By  this  time,  embryos  had  developed 
into  planulae  competent  to  metamorphose  (Plickert  et 
ai,  1988).  Metamorphosis  was  induced  by  ionic  imbal- 
ance (Spindler  and  Muller,  1972;  Weis  and  Buss,  1987). 
Competent  planulae  were  transferred  to  a  53  mA/  CsCl 
solution  in  seawater.  After  approximately  4  h,  planulae 
were  placed  on  glass  cover  slips  in  seawater-filled  six-well 
plates  ( 1  planula  per  well).  Attachment  and  metamor- 
phosis occurred  within  2  days.  Six  plates  per  cross  (36 
planulae  total)  were  metamorphosed.  Colonies  were  fed 
3-day-old  brine  shrimp  nauplii,  followed  by  a  complete 
water  change  each  day. 

Colonies  were  maintained  in  an  incubator  at  12.5°C 
for  50  days  (to  a  mean  size  of  1 1  feeding  polyps).  The 
temperature  conditions  were  chosen  to  reflect  the  ambient 
temperatures  in  Long  Island  Sound  during  the  spring  and 
early  summer  (Yund  et  al..  1987).  At  this  point  in  the 
seasonal  cycle,  sexual  reproduction,  recruitment,  and  in- 
traspecific  competition  occur  at  high  frequencies  in  this 
area  (Buss  and  Yund,  1988).  The  duration  of  the  exper- 
imental period  was  chosen  for  the  purpose  of  assessing 
colony  shape  at  small  colony  sizes.  Hydractinia  planulae 
display  site-specific  settlement  on  shells,  hence  the  vast 
majority  of  intraspecific  competitive  encounters  occur  at 
small  colony  sizes  (Yund  et  al.,  1987;  Buss  and  Yund, 
1988;  Yund  and  Parker,  1989;  see  discussion  below). 

Using  the  protocols  described  above,  colonies  were 
measured  for  area  and  perimeter  as  soon  as  primary  polyps 
and  stolons  developed  after  metamorphosis  (<5  days). 
Each  colony  was  measured  at  weekly  intervals  up  to  an 
age  of  50  days  (25-50%  of  the  colonies  of  each  cross  failed 
to  survive  to  this  age).  We  analyzed  the  data  using  quan- 
titative genetic  techniques  (Falconer.  1981).  Because  of 
the  small  size  of  the  laboratory  population,  we  suggest 
only  very  limited  interpretation  of  our  results  with  regard 
to  the  natural  population  of  Hydractinia.  Rather,  we  in- 
tended to  gain  further  insight  into  the  results  suggested 


by  the  clonal  repeatibility  experiments;  is  shape  largely 
genetically  determined,  i.e..  does  shape  variation  have  a 
large  broad-sense  heritability,  and  further,  is  there  any 
evidence  that  the  broad-sense  heritability  of  shape  vari- 
ation in  this  laboratory  population  is  due  to  narrow-sense 
heritability?  Analyses  were  done  on  initial  perimeter/ 
iarea  (age  <5  days),  on  mean  perimeter/} 'area  (for  each 
colony,  all  shape  measures  up  to  age  50  days  were  aver- 
aged, and  this  mean  value  was  used  as  the  outcome),  and 
on  final  perimeter/} 'area  (age  =  50  days).  These  three 
comparisons  correspond  to  before,  during,  and  after  sto- 
lonal  mat  formation. 

Although  our  goals  were  somewhat  different  from  typ- 
ical quantitative  genetic  studies  (cf..  Falconer,  1981),  we 
used  standard  methods  to  examine  the  covariance  of  full 
sibs  and  the  covariance  of  half  sibs.  Specifically,  the  be- 
tween-female parent  component  of  variance  (i.e.,  ak.mai^2- 
the  variance  between  the  means  of  the  half-sib  families) 
estimates  COVHS  and  measures  additive  genetic  variance 
(i.e..  narrow-sense  heritability,  provided  maternal  effects 
are  slight).  The  between-male  parent  component  of  vari- 
ance, <rmi,/t.,2.  estimates  COVFS  -  COVHS  and  measures  a 
combination  of  additive  and  non-additive  genetic  variance 
(i.e..  broad-sense  heritability,  provided  environmental  ef- 
fects are  slight).  Insight  into  additive  and  non-additive 
genetic  variance  can  thus  be  obtained  from  a  nested  anal- 
ysis of  variance.  The  F-ratio  of  the  male-parent  mean 
square  to  the  within-brood  mean  square  will  measure  ad- 
ditive and  non-additive  genetic  variance,  while  the  F-ratio 
of  the  female-parent  mean  square  to  the  male-parent  mean 
square  will  measure  additive  genetic  variance  (see  results 
below).  We  focus  on  qualitative  interpretations  of  the 
analysis  of  variance  rather  than  exact  calculations  of  her- 
itabilities because  of  the  small  size  of  the  laboratory  pop- 
ulation and  the  limited  goals  of  our  breeding  study  (see 
Mitchell-Olds.  1986;  Via,  1988). 

To  properly  gauge  the  inheritance  of  shape,  we  at- 
tempted to  reduce  environmental  effects  in  several  ways. 
First,  because  we  expected  a  priori  that  non-additive  ge- 
netic variance  would  be  large  relative  to  additive  genetic 
variance  (i.e..  COVFS  >  COVHS,  see  discussion  below), 
each  female  parent  was  mated  to  two  male  parents.  Thus, 
any  maternal  or  cytoplasmic  effects  (see  discussion  in 
Mazer.  1987)  will  inflate  the  covariance  of  the  half  sibs 
and  inflate  our  estimate  of  additive  genetic  variance.  Sec- 
ond, because  matings  were  initiated  at  slightly  different 
times  and  because  between-mating  environmental  vari- 
ation could  inflate  the  covariance  of  the  full  sibs,  envi- 
ronmental conditions  were  closely  controlled.  In  addition 
to  incubation  at  a  constant  temperature,  seawater  chem- 
istry was  monitored  weekly,  and  nitrates  and  nitrites  were 
maintained  at  low  levels  (<9.0  ppm  and  <0.01  ppm,  re- 
spectively). Salinity  was  maintained  at  =26  ppt.  Any 


398 


N.  W.  BLACKSTONE  AND  L.  W.  BUSS 


variation  in  environmental  conditions  was  slight  and 
showed  no  systematic  trend  over  the  time  course  of  the 
experiment.  Finally,  colony  position  effects  were  assessed. 
Stacks  of  culture  plates  (6  per  mating)  were  kept  on  a 
single  shelf  in  an  incubator  and  positions  were  varied  daily 
in  a  random  manner.  Individual  plates,  however,  were 
kept  in  descending  order  (1-6),  and  culture  wells  were 
also  in  fixed  positions.  Because  there  was  only  one  colony 
per  well,  wells  were  pooled  into  left  wells,  center  wells, 
and  right  wells  based  on  their  positions  in  the  six-well 
plate.  The  complete  analysis  was  thus  well  position  nested 
within  plate,  plate  nested  within  male  parent,  and  male 
parent  nested  within  female  parent.  This  analysis  was  car- 
ried out  for  intial  and  average  shape  measures.  By  the  age 
of  50  days,  the  25-50%  mortality  for  each  cross  rendered 
the  analysis  of  well  position  effects  and  plate  effects  un- 
reliable because  of  missing  values,  and  the  pooled  within- 
broods  mean  square  was  used  as  the  error  variance. 

Further  insight  into  environmental  effects  was  gained 
by  two  additional  experiments.  First,  for  one  of  the  ma- 
ternal half-sib  families,  three  50-day-old  offspring  from 
each  paternal  cross  were  explanted  onto  snail  shells  oc- 
cupied by  hermit  crabs  and  cultured  in  the  40-liter  aquaria 
until  each  colony  covered  its  shell  and  was  fully  mature. 
The  6  colonies  were  then  compared  using  the  method  of 
clonal  repeatibility  described  above,  i.e.,  5  explants  from 
each  colony  were  grown  on  cover  slips  in  a  floating  rack 
at  16°C  and  perimeter/} 'area  was  measured  at  10  days 
after  explanting.  These  shape  measures  were  then  com- 
pared to  the  measures  made  on  the  colonies  in  their  first 
50  days  of  growth.  Second,  two  colonies  from  each  of 
three  crosses  were  grown  in  the  six-well  plates  as  described 
above  until  they  grew  to  the  edge  of  the  coverslip  (60- 
120  days).  Measures  of  perimeter/^  area  were  made  at 
roughly  weekly  intervals. 

Results 

Growth  morphology  and  shape 

For  the  242  50-day-old  Hydractinia  colonies  measured, 
indices  of  peripheral  stolon  development  (total  colony 
perimeter/stolonal  mat  perimeter  and  total  colony  area/ 
stolonal  mat  area)  correlate  highly  with  total  colony  pe- 
rimeter divided  by  the  square  root  of  total  colony  area  (rs 
=  0.95  and  0.91,  respectively).  Because  the  correlated 
variables  contain  similar  measures  of  total  colony  size 
(total  perimeter,  total  area,  or  the  square  root  of  total 
area),  the  possibility  of  autocorrelation  exists.  For  two 
reasons,  however,  the  underlying  structure  of  the  data 
suggests  that  autocorrelation  has  negligible  effects. 

First,  adjusting  the  correlated  ratios  to  remove  similar 
variables  from  each  does  not  alter  the  correlations.  Sto- 
lonal mat  perimeter  is  highly  correlated  with  the  square 


root  of  total  colony  area  (r>  =  0.94;  note  that  r,  is  insen- 
sitive to  transformations  of  the  correlated  variables  so  that 
the  correlation  of  stolonal  mat  perimeter  and  total  colony 
area  is  also  0.94).  Additionally,  total  colony  perimeter  is 
highly  correlated  with  the  ratio  (total  colony  area)3/:/sto- 
lonal  mat  area  (r,  =  0.92). 

Second,  either  measure  of  stolonal  mat  size  (perimeter 
or  area)  shows  a  high  correlation  with  total  colony  area 
(r,  =  0.94  and  0.92,  respectively)  but  much  weaker  cor- 
relations with  total  colony  perimeter  (rs  =  0.63  and  0.53, 
respectively).  Stolonal  mat  size  is  thus  indicative  of  total 
colony  area,  but  less  so  of  total  colony  perimeter.  These 
results  are  consistent  with  the  stolonal  mat  showing  cir- 
cular growth  in  these  small  colonies,  and  deviations  from 
circular  growth  being  caused  by  peripheral  stolons. 

Shape  variation  in  Podocoryne  and  Hydractinia 

Measures  of  perimeter/^ 'area  for  both  7  and  14  days 
after  explanting  show  that  Hydractinia  has  more  circular 
shapes  than  Podocoryne  (Table  I).  To  analyze  these  data, 
a  natural  logarithmic  transformation  was  done  to  better 
meet  the  assumptions  of  the  analysis  of  variance.  The  log- 
transformed  data  were  first  analyzed  to  assess  the  effects 
of  the  positions  of  the  colonies  within  the  racks,  i.e.,  rep- 
licates nested  within  species  nested  within  positions  nested 
within  racks.  Because  of  the  different  numbers  of  replicates 


Table  I 

Shape  variation  in  15  strains  of  Podocoryne  and  Hydractinia3 


Podocoryne 


Hydractinia 


Strain"      n 


Age  7 


Age  14 


Age  7 


Age  14 


1 

3 

17.87 

1.03 

21.44 

0.99 

5 

9.03 

0.58 

17.46 

0.70 

2 

5 

13.67 

3.96 

17.06 

3.62 

5 

4.17 

0.10 

5.40 

0.43 

3 

3 

7.20 

2.08 

15.70 

3.52 

3 

3.83 

0.08 

3.94  0.08 

4 

5 

18.54 

1.87 

25.38 

1.94 

5 

5.12 

0.68 

5.44 

1.06 

5 

2 

15.24 

1.67 

26.26  0.86 

5 

4.330.10 

4.05 

0.13 

6 

4 

23.78 

1.92 

23.97 

1.89 

5 

7.23 

0.89 

15.46 

2.70 

7 

3 

20.35 

1.40 

33.43 

2.09 

4 

4.44 

0.33 

5.18 

0.67 

8 

5 

19.06 

3.46 

30.62 

1.10 

5 

9.92 

2.48 

15.12 

2.33 

9 

5 

20.72 

1.79 

20.16 

0.96 

4 

4.52 

0.23 

6.55 

0.87 

10 

4 

22.04 

2.49 

26.50 

1.85 

4 

6.56 

0.52 

11.72 

0.47 

11 

5 

22.59 

1.25 

23.22 

2.55 

-> 

8.14 

3.47 

10.53 

5.24 

12 

4 

13.23 

2.08 

21.39 

2.81 

5 

5.63 

0.52 

10.46 

1.48 

13 

5 

21.54 

0.83 

22.63 

1.33 

5 

7.45 

0.74 

10.72 

1.22 

14 

5 

17.87 

1.69 

21.72 

3.30 

4 

10.92 

1.71 

15.75 

1.03 

15 

4 

10.63 

2.43 

19.15 

0.73 

5 

11.07 

1.31 

19.81 

1.06 

'  Shape  measures  are  perimeter/]/ area:  means  and  standard  errors  are 
shown  for  n  replicates  of  each  strain,  7  and  14  days  after  explanting. 

b  For  Podocoryne,  strains  3,  7, 9,  1 3.  and  1 5  are  females,  and  for  strain 
3  at  age  14  n  =  2.  For  Hydractinia.  strains  3,  5,  12,  14,  and  15  are 
females,  and  for  strain  14  at  age  14  n  =  3. 


HYDRACT1NIID  HYDROID  SHAPE 


399 


(some  replicates  were  lost  due  to  mortality,  see  Table  I), 
the  nested  ANOVAs  were  unbalanced,  although  exami- 
nation of  the  coefficients  of  the  variance  components  in- 
dicated a  high  reliability  of  the  F-tests  carried  out  (see 
discussion  in  Sokal  and  Rohlf,  1981).  At  day  7,  there  is 
a  strong  effect  of  species  (F  =  26.95.  d.f.  =  15,  98,  P 
«  0.001).  but  no  effect  of  position  (F  =  0.06,  d.f.  =  12, 
15,  P>  0.99)  or  rack  (F  =  1.05.  d.f.  =  2,  12,  P>  0.35). 
Similarly,  at  day  14,  species  shows  a  strong  effect  (F 
=  26.96,  d.f.  ==  15,  96,  P  <s  0.001),  while  position  (F 
=  0.19,  d.f.  =  12,  15,  P>  0.99)  and  rack  (F  =  0.70.  d.f. 
=  2,  1 2,  P  >  0.50)  do  not.  Based  on  this  analysis,  position 
effects  were  dropped  from  the  model:  this  allowed  in- 
cluding the  strain  effects  (i.e..  replicates  within  strains 
within  species  within  racks,  see  Table  II).  Again,  because 
of  the  different  numbers  of  replicates,  this  ANOVA  is  also 
unbalanced.  Examination  of  the  coefficients  of  the  vari- 
ance components  (Table  II)  suggests  that  F-ratios  should 
be  reliable;  in  particular,  the  F-ratio  assessing  the  effects 
of  racks  (i.e.,  MSmiA7MSwat,,)  is  highly  reliable,  while  the 
F-ratio  assessing  the  effects  of  species  (i.e..  MS,,,,,,,.,/ 
MSS,,,,,,,S)  is  slightly  conservative.  The  results  are  similar 
to  the  first  analysis;  there  is  no  effect  of  racks  (F  =  0.02, 
d.f.  =  2,  3,  P  >  0.95)  and  a  strong  effect  of  species  (F 
=  20.45,  d.f.  =  3,  24,  P  <<  0.001 ).  Further,  strains  within 
species  show  significant  variation  (F  =  4.99,  d.f.  =  24,  98, 
P  <$  0.001 ).  Results  at  14  days  are  similar;  racks  show  no 
effect  (F  =  0.03,  d.f.  =  2.  3,  P  >  0.95),  while  species  (F 
=  1 2.94,  d.f.  =  3,  24,  P  «  0.00 1 )  and  strains  (F  =  8. 1 1 , 
d.f.  =  24,  96.  P  <t  0.001)  show  strong  effects. 

In  addition  to  shape  differences,  Podocoryne  exhibits 
faster  growth  rates  than  Hydractinia  (Table  III).  Specific 
growth  rates  of  polyps  show  no  effect  of  racks  (F  =  0.21, 
d.f.  =  2,  3,  P  >  0.80),  but  significant  effects  of  species  (F 
=  11. 75.  d.f.  =  3,  24,  P<s  0.001)  and  of  strains  (F  =  3.79, 
d.f.  =  24,  96,  P  <£  0.001).  Similarly,  specific  growth  rates 
of  colony  areas  show  no  effect  of  racks  (F  =  0.35,  d.f.  =  2, 
3,  P  >  0.70),  a  moderate  effect  of  species  (F  =  6.70,  d.f. 


Table  II 

AnulvMs  i>l  variance  table  for  log-transformed  shape  measures  at  day  7 
ot  the  i'lonul  repeatibility  experiment 


Table  III 

Speeifie  growth  rales  fur  15  strains  ot  Podocoryne  and  Hydractinia3 


Source 


Mean 
d.f.   square       Composition  of  mean  square 


Between  racks  2  0.104  a em,,2  +  4.00(7  „,„,„-  +  20.00<7J(,m, 

+  40.00(7  ,Mc 

Between  species  within  3  10.897  afrmr2  +  4.02(7  ,,ra,,,:  -I-  20.08(7  ,,,,,„, 

racks 

Between  strains  within  24  0.497  at,rm2  +  4.22a,,ra,,:2 

species 

Between  replicates  98  0.099  o-,,rror2 


Podocoryne 


Hvdraciinta 


Strain 

n 

Area 

Polyp 

n 

Area 

Polyp 

1 

3 

0.21  1  0.01 

0.1970.01 

5 

0.181  0.01 

0.1840.01 

2 

5 

0.181  0.05 

0.1690.02 

5 

0.0340.01 

0.00    0.00 

3 

2 

0.1100.09 

0.07  1  0.07 

3 

0.059  0.06 

0.013  0.04 

4 

5 

0.2260.01 

0.1660.02 

5 

0.0380.01 

0.035  0.02 

5 

2 

0.1300.06 

0.069  0.04 

5 

0.1020.01 

0.1130.02 

6 

4 

0.195  0.01 

0.  1  38  0.02 

5 

0.092  0.03 

0.074  0.03 

7 

3 

0.2030.01 

0.1700.03 

4 

0.1240.01 

0.1230.01 

8 

S 

0.2200.01 

0.1970.01 

5 

0.1760.02 

0.101  0.02 

9 

5 

0.1840.03 

0.  1  7  1  0.02 

4 

0.0630.01 

0.07  1  0.03 

10 

4 

0.  1  74  0.02 

0.1660.03 

4 

0.113  0.03 

0.1000.02 

1  1 

5 

0.191  0.0  1 

0.2150.01 

2 

0.050  0.03 

0.0190.08 

12 

4 

0.101  0.06 

0.220  0.03 

5 

0.1000.01 

0.079  0.01 

13 

5 

0.1920.02 

0.2070.01 

5 

0.1020.01 

0.1480.02 

14 

5 

0.1120.03 

0.181  0.02 

3 

0.1440.01 

0.042  0.03 

15 

4 

0.1580.03 

0.1370.02 

5 

0.1920.01 

0.079  0.02 

a  Specific  growth  rates  ( I/day)  for  total  colony  area  and  polyp  number 
for  7  to  14  days  after  explanting;  strains  are  designated  by  the  same 
numbers  as  in  Table  I.  and  means  and  standard  errors  are  shown  for  n 
replicates  of  each  strain. 


=  3,  24,  P<  0.01)  and  a  strong  effect  of  strains  (F  =  2.90, 
d.f.  =  24,  96,  P<Z  0.001). 

Breeding  studies 

For  the  breeding  experiments  conducted  with  Hydrac- 
tiniu.  the  shapes  of  the  colonies  initially  and  at  50  days 
(Table  IV)  show  little  correlation  (rs  =  -0.06,  P  >  0.30, 
for  all  242  50-day-old  colonies).  This  likely  results  from 
growth  changes  associated  with  stolonal  mat  formation 
(Fig.  1 ).  Despite  such  variation,  analyses  of  initial,  average, 
and  final  colony  shape  all  suggest  a  highly  significant  effect 
of  the  male  parent  and  a  non-significant  effect  of  the  fe- 
male parent.  For  initial  colony  shape,  there  is  no  effect 
of  the  female  parent  (F  =  0.98,  d.f.  =  4,  5,  P  >  0.45),  a 
strong  effect  of  the  male  parent  (F  =  7.39,  d.f.  =  5,  50,  P 
<§  0.00 1 ),  and  no  effect  of  either  plate  (F  =  0.85,  d.f.  =  50, 
119,  P  >  0.70),  or  well  position  (F  =  0.95,  d.f.  =  1 19. 
173,  P  >  0.60).  Similarly,  for  average  colony  shape  there 
is  no  effect  of  the  female  parent  (F  =  0.44,  d.f.  =  4,  5,  P 
>  0.75)  and  a  strong  effect  of  the  male  parent  (F  =  15.21, 
d.f.  =  5,  50,  P  «  0.001).  Again,  there  was  no  effect  of 
either  plate  (F  =  0.88,  d.f.  =  50,  1 19,  P  >  0.65)  or  well 
position  (F  =  1.04,  d.f.  =  119,  173,  P>  0.40).  For  both 
initial  and  average  shape  analyses,  the  coefficients  of  the 
variance  components  (Table  V)  indicate  a  high  reliability 
of  the  F-ratios.  For  final  shape  measures,  missing  values 
(because  of  mortality)  rendered  the  analysis  of  position 


400 


N.  W.  BLACKSTONE  AND  L.  W.  BUSS 


Table  IV 

/ '  ve  valislicsfor  the  10  crosses  used  in  the  breeding  st 


o) 


Initial 

Average 

Final 

Parents* 

n 

Shape 

n 

Shape 

n 

Shape 

Polyp 

15  X  11 

36 

5.86  0.25 

36 

8.36  0.47 

23 

1  1.44  0.91 

19.6  3.1 

15  x  13 

36 

6.61  0.39 

36 

6.71  0.40 

28 

6.75  0.54 

8.3  1.2 

12X7 

36 

7.26  0.30 

36 

6.170.20 

28 

4.99  0.38 

11.1  0.9 

12  X  2 

36 

7.08  0.36 

36 

7.85  0.28 

23 

8.98  0.70 

5.4  1.0 

5X8 

36 

7.770.42 

36 

7.98  0.29 

21 

6.78  0.73 

12.2  1.4 

5X9 

36 

7.90  0.35 

36 

6.87  0.30 

19 

6.76  0.68 

1  1.9  1.4 

14  x  1 

36 

8.56  0.46 

36 

8.74  0.34 

31 

6.590.61 

19.7  1.1 

14  x  10 

28 

5.55  0.23 

28 

5.63  0.22 

17 

6.74  0.76 

19.52.4 

3x4 

36 

7.1  1  0.37 

36 

6.28  0.27 

27 

4.13  0.14 

8.4  0.9 

3X6 

36 

6.09  0.34 

36 

6.180.34 

25 

5.64  0.56 

8.0  1.2 

"Sample  sizes  (n).  means  and  standard  errors  for  shape  measures 
(perimeter/lfarea)  from  initial  colonies  (primary  polyps  <  5  days  old), 
average  colonies  (for  each  colony,  all  shape  measures  up  to  50  days  were 
averaged;  this  mean  value  was  used  for  the  descriptive  statistics),  and 
final  colonies  (50  days  old).  Data  on  polyp  number  (mean  and  standard 
error)  is  also  presented  for  the  final  colonies. 

b  Numbers  designating  female  parent  and  male  parent  respectively: 
the  numbers  correspond  to  the  strains  of  Hydraeiinia  from  Tables  I  and 
III 


effects  unreliable;  nevertheless,  using  the  pooled  within- 
broods  mean  square  as  the  error  variance,  the  data  suggest 
a  non-significant  effect  of  the  female  parent  (F  :=  1.23, 
d.f.  =  4,  5,  P>  0.40)  and  a  large  effect  of  the  male  parent 
(F  ==  1 1.3,  d.f.  =  5,  232,  P  «  0.001).  Overall,  the  slight 
effect  of  the  female  parent  (i.e..  a  non-significant  covari- 
ance  of  the  half  sibs)  indicates  that  ff(em.d\fi  is  relatively 
small  and  that  both  maternal  effects  and  additive  genetic 
variance  are  correspondingly  small.  On  the  other  hand, 
the  large  effect  of  the  male  parent  suggests  a  large  co- 
variance  of  the  full  sibs.  a  relatively  large  (7maies2.  and  likely 
a  large  non-additive  genetic  variance,  given  the  closely 
controlled  environmental  conditions.  The  interpretation 
of  these  results  should  be  limited  to  the  small  laboratory 
population  on  which  the  breeding  studies  were  based  (see 
Discussion). 

While  environmental  effects  could  not  be  tested  directly 
with  this  experimental  design,  the  six  offspring  raised  on 
hermit  crab  shells  and  then  compared  using  clonal  re- 
peatibility  allow  an  assessment  of  the  sensitivity  of  Hy- 
dractinia  colony  morphology  to  environmental  circum- 
stances. These  colonies  were  from  crosses  15x11  and 
15X13  (see  Table  IV);  Figure  3  shows  the  shape  measures 
for  the  50-day  ontogenies  of  the  young  colonies.  After 
these  young  colonies  were  grown  to  maturity  on  hermit 
crab  shells,  explants  were  made;  Figure  3  also  shows  the 
mean  shape  measures  for  five  10-day-old  replicates  from 
each  of  the  mature  colonies  (means  and  standard  errors 


primary 
polyp 


c) 


stolonal 
mat 


polyp 


Figure  I.  Rough  schemata  of  (a)  a  primary  polyp  of  a  hydractiniid 
hydroid.  (b)  a  small  Podocoryne  colony,  and  small  Hydraeiinia  colonies 
of  the  (c)  "mat  type."  with  few  peripheral  stolons,  and  the  (d)  "net  type," 
with  more  peripheral  stolons.  Colonies  are  drawn  as  if  encrusting  the 
surface  of  the  page;  polyps  would  project  up  out  of  the  plane  of  the  paper. 
In  the  stolonal  mat  (the  central  portion  of  the  Hydraeiinia  colonies  rep- 
resented by  the  stipled  pattern),  the  spaces  between  the  stolons  are  filled 
with  tissue;  thus  these  stolons  are  fused  together,  while  the  peripheral 
stolons  outside  the  stipled  area  (and  those  in  Podocoryne)  are  unfused. 
Colonies  are  drawn  to  roughly  the  same  scale;  stolon  width  is  approxi- 
mately 70  microns. 


for  the  5  replicates  of  each  colony  are  adjacent  to  the 
symbol  for  the  50-day  shape  measure  for  that  colony). 
Comparing  these  shape  data  generated  by  different  meth- 
ods suggests  that,  despite  different  culture  conditions,  both 
methods  generate  roughly  similar  data  for  the  same  col- 


Table  v 

Analysis  ot  \-ananee  table  lor  the  analysis  of  I  he  natural  logarithms 
ol  average  shape  measures  for  each  colour 

Mean 
Source  d.f.    square       Composition  of  mean  square 

Between  female  parent       4    0.395    n  ,.„„,-  +  1.93d  ,,(.,,:  +  5.11  a  pia,,2 

+  34.60(7  ma,es-  +  6<).2\afemalfs2 
Between  male  parent          5    0.890    cr,.rrar:  +  1.94cr,,,,.,,:  +  5.78trp/u,,,2 


Between  plate  50    0.059    afm,2  +  1.95<fHrf/2  +  5.82ff,,to,: 

Between  well  position      119    0.066    amf-  +  1.96(i,,,.;/: 
Within  well  position        173    0.064    aern,,2 


HYDRACTINI1D  HVDROID  SHAPE 


401 


24  0- 


200- 


~   16-0- 

E 


cr 

hJ 


120 


cr 

UJ 
Q- 


80- 


4  0 


00 


186 


93        • 

1^ 

•  mm 


9 .5 


-JV 

-?  f  5-4 


00 


075 


1-5 


2  25 


30 


AREAImm2) 


Figure  2.  Perimeter  versus  area  plots  for  two  sibling  Hydractinia 
colonies  measured  every  other  day  for  the  first  5  weeks  of  ontogeny. 
Camera  lucida  tracings  (not  to  scale)  and  shape  measures  (perim- 
eterffarea)  are  shown  for  some  of  the  data  points.  While  shapes  fluctuate 
as  stolons  branch  and  fuse,  the  two  colonies  exhibit  distinct  trajectories 
in  perimeter  versus  area  space. 

ony,  and,  in  particular,  either  method  shows  the  differ- 
ences between  the  crosses.  At  50  days,  cross  15X11  pro- 
duced significantly  more  irregular  shapes  than  cross  15 
X  13  (Table  IV);  analysis  of  the  clonal  repeatibility  data 
shows  the  same  pattern.  If  the  log-transformed  shape  data 
are  analyzed  as  replicates  nested  within  crosses  nested 
within  positions,  there  is  an  effect  of  cross  (F  =  7.9,  d.f. 
=  3,  24,  P  <  0.001),  but  no  effect  of  position  (F  =  0.05, 
d.f.  =  2,  3,  P  >  0.90).  Analyzing  the  data  as  replicates 
nested  within  strains  nested  within  crosses  provides  a 
similar  result  (no  significant  effect  of  strains  F  =  0.92,  d.f. 
=  4,  24,  P  >  0.45.  but  a  significant  effect  of  crosses  F 
=  22. 7,  d.f.  =  1.4,  P<  0.01).  In  either  case,  cross  15  X  11 
exhibits  significantly  more  irregular  shapes  than  cross  1 5 
X  1 3.  This  supports  the  findings  of  the  breeding  experi- 
ment and  suggests  that  the  differences  between  crosses  are 
not  the  result  of  some  undetected  environmental  factor 
varying  over  time. 

The  6  colonies  from  3  different  crosses  (12  X  7,  5  X  8, 
and  3  X  4  in  Table  IV)  which  were  grown  beyond  50  days 
(Fig.  4)  show  some  variation  in  colony  shape,  but  also 
suggest  that  differences  among  crosses  are  maintained  at 


larger  sizes.  For  instance,  at  1 10  days,  the  4  colonies  which 
had  not  yet  reached  the  edges  of  the  cover  slips  suggest 
the  same  differences  in  shape,  which  were  apparent  for 
the  complete  crosses  at  50  days  (Table  IV;  for  perim- 
eter/^ area,  5  X  8  >  1 2  X  7  >  3  X  4). 


Discussion 

These  results  have  implications  with  regard  to  mor- 
phological variation  in  hydractiniid  hydroidsand  in  other 
clonal  taxa  as  well.  We  discuss  ( 1 )  the  biological  basis  of 
shape  in  hydractiniid  hydroids,  (2)  the  implications  of  the 
Hydractinia  breeding  studies,  (3)  the  general  phenomenon 
of  heterochrony  in  hydractiniid  hydroids,  and  (4)  the  rel- 
evance of  these  results  to  other  clonal  taxa. 


172(09) 


15- 


10- 


5- 


84(1  8) 


10 


20  30 

AGE  (DAYS) 


40 


50 


Figure  3.  Shapes  (pernnelcr/^area)  of  6  colonies  from  one  of  the 
maternal  half-sib  families  (3  from  15  x  11,  shown  by  circles,  3  from  15 
•  13.  shown  by  squares:  the  crosses  are  designated  as  in  Table  IV)  for 
the  first  50  days  of  growth  (lines  connect  points  for  each  individual). 
These  colonies  were  grown  to  maturity  on  hermit  crab  shells  and  then 
explanted  and  measured  for  shape  again.  Numbers  adjacent  to  the  symbol 
for  the  50-day  shape  value  show  the  means,  with  standard  errors  in 
parentheses,  for  5  replicates  of  each  colony  10  days  after  explanting. 
Despite  differences  in  culture  conditions,  the  clonal  repeatibility  shape 
measures  and  the  shape  measures  for  the  first  50  days  both  suggest  that 
colonies  from  15-  11  have  more  irregular  shapes  than  those  from  1 5 
x  13. 


402 


N.  W.  BLACKSTONE  AND  L.  W.  BUSS 


40  80 

AGE (DAYS) 


120 


Figure  4.  Two  colonies  from  each  of  3  crosses  (5x8,  circles;  12 
x  7,  squares;  and  3x4,  triangles;  the  crosses  are  designated  as  in  Table 
IV)  were  grown  beyond  50  days  on  glass  cover  slips  and  measured  for 
shape  (perimeter /y  area).  Lines  connect  values  for  each  colony:  2  colonies 
reached  the  edges  of  the  cover  slips  in  <80  days;  the  remaining  4  were 
grown  for  over  100  days.  While  shapes  fluctuate,  at  1  10  days  these  4 
colonies  suggest  shape  differences  which  were  apparent  for  all  colonies 
of  each  cross  at  50  days  (Table  IV;  5  x  8  >  12  X  7  >  3  X  4). 


The  biological  basis  of  shape 

The  results  presented  show  a  strong  correlation  between 
measures  of  peripheral  stolon  development  and  pcrim- 
eter/^area  in  Hydractinia.  Previous  work  (McFadden  et 
al..  1984;  Yund,  1987;  Buss  and  Grosberg,  1990)  suggests 
that  peripheral  stolon  development  is  correlated 
with  competitive  ability  in  Hydractinia.  Thus,  shape  as  mea- 
sured by  perimeter/} 'area  will  show  a  similar  correlation. 
Further,  Podocoryne  shows  more  irregular  shapes  than 
Hydractinia  and  thus  greater  peripheral  stolon  develop- 
ment; this  result  agrees  with  its  competitive  dominance 
over  Hydractinia  in  laboratory  studies  (McFadden,  1986). 
The  correlations  of  shape  with  competitive  ability  can 
make  measures  of  shape  useful  to  biologists,  but  clearly 
shape  differences  are  not  causally  related  to  competitive 
ability  (see  discussion  below).  Rather,  shape,  competitive 
ability,  and  peripheral  stolon  development  are  likely  cor- 
related consequences  of  the  underlying  dynamics  of 
growth  in  these  hydroids. 

Shape  measures  bear  a  clearly  interpretable  relation- 
ship to  these  growth  dynamics.  Examining  Figure  1  suggests 
that  perimeter/M area  will  measure  the  degree  to  which 


stolons  extend  from  a  central  ring  stolon  or  network  of 
ring  stolons.  Stolons  encase  the  gastrovascular  canals;  the 
combined  actions  of  stolons  and,  in  particular,  muscular 
polyps  drive  the  gastrovascular  fluid  through  the  canals 
and  nourish  the  colony  (see  Schierwater  et  al..  in  press). 
Since  the  gastrovascular  system  is  closed,  peripheral  sto- 
lons are  essentially  dead-end  channels,  and  a  considerable 
pressure  is  likely  necessary  to  supply  these  stolons  with 
fluid.  Contrast  this  to  the  situation  in  a  ring  stolon  or 
network  of  ring  stolons  well-supplied  by  polyps:  here,  flow 
can  proceed  from  polyp  to  polyp  with  relatively  little  ex- 
ertion of  pressure.  Hence,  the  amount  of  peripheral  stolons 
and  the  degree  of  non-circularity  of  colony  shape  are  likely 
to  be  measures  of  the  ability  of  the  colony  to  maintain 
the  energetic  and  physiological  costs  of  supplying  these 
peripheral  elements  with  gastrovascular  flow. 

Hydractinia  breeding  studies 

Generally,  the  clonal  repeatibility  experiments  and  the 
breeding  experiments  produced  compatible  results  for  the 
15  strains  of  Hydractinia,  despite  differences  in  culture 
conditions.  For  instance,  the  fastest  and  slowest  growing 
strains  ( 1  and  2,  measured  by  polyp  specific  growth  rates) 
produce  the  fastest  and  slowest  growing  offspring  (14X1 
and  12X2,  measured  by  polyp  number  at  50  days),  the 
strain  with  the  most  irregular  shape  ( 1 5)  in  one  cross  pro- 
duced offspring  with  the  most  irregular  shapes  ( 15  X  11), 
and  strains  with  nearly  circular  shapes  ( 3  and  4)  produced 
nearly  circular  offspring  (3  X  4).  The  somewhat  circular 
shape  of  colony  2  seems  at  variance  with  the  irregular 
shapes  of  12  X  2;  however,  this  likely  indicates  the  limi- 
tations of  comparing  developing  colonies  to  clonal  ex- 
plants.  Colony  2  shows  extremely  slow  growth  as  do  its 
offspring  ( 12  X  2).  It  is  likely  that  this  slow  growth  reflects 
equally  slow  development  of  adult  colony  form  and  or- 
ganization. The  irregular  shapes  of  young  12X2  colonies 
may  indicate  a  slow  transition  from  early  colony  devel- 
opment to  adult  morphology. 

Despite  the  agreement  of  the  results  of  the  clonal  re- 
peatibility experiments  and  the  breeding  experiments,  the 
latter  can  provide  classes  of  information  which  are  not 
available  from  the  former.  While  only  a  small  breeding 
study  was  carried  out  here,  it  is,  to  our  knowledge,  the 
first  example  of  carefully  controlled  crosses  for  a  clonal 
organism.  To  stimulate  further  such  work,  we  will  discuss 
the  general  value  of  such  data.  Clonal  repeatibility  studies 
demonstrate  a  significant  broad-sense  heritability  of  the 
shape  variation  (the  strain-within-species  effect  in  the 
ANOVAs).  The  breeding  studies  not  only  demonstrate  a 
significant  broad-sense  heritability  (the  effect  of  the  male 
parent  in  the  ANOVAs),  but  also  provide  information  on 
the  sorts  of  genetic  variation  that  constitute  this  broad- 


HYDRACTINIID  HYDROID  SHAPE 


403 


sense  heritability.  For  the  three  analyses  (initial,  average, 
and  final  shape  measures),  MS/l.,mj/t.v/MS,,,,,/(.,  is  roughly  1 
(0.98, 0.44,  and  1 .23  respectively,  see  Results)  and  in  each 
case  is  non-significant.  This  suggests  that  the  covariance 
of  the  half-sibs  is  small,  that  is,  progeny  from  the  same 
maternal  half-sib  family  are  not  appreciably  more  similar 
to  each  other  than  to  unrelated  progeny.  On  the  other 
hand,  the  covariance  of  the  full-sibs  is  high  (hence  the 
large  effect  of  the  male  parent).  If  environmental  effects 
are  slight,  these  results  suggest  that  non-additive  genetic 
variance  constitutes  the  bulk  of  the  broad-sense  heritability 
of  shape  in  this  small  laboratory  population.  This  result 
is  bolstered  by  the  additional  study  of  one  of  the  maternal 
half-sib  families  (progeny  of  female  15,  i.e.,  crosses  15 
X  1 1  and  15  X  13).  Using  clonal  repeatibility,  this  study 
shows  that  the  difference  between  the  full-sib  families  does 
not  depend  on  some  undetected  environmental  factor. 

Thus,  it  is  possible  that  within  this  small  laboratory 
population  of  Hydractinia.  the  genes  controlling  shape 
variation  show  high  levels  of  dominance  and  epistasis. 
This  result  is  supported  by  examining  the  data  qualita- 
tively. In  Table  IV,  paternal  full-sib  families  are  very  sim- 
ilar (e.g.,  note  the  low  standard  errors).  Nevertheless, 
within  a  particular  maternal  half-sib  family,  full-sib  fam- 
ilies are  often  very  different  (e.g.,  progeny  of  females  3, 
12,  and  15).  Thus,  the  expression  of  the  shape  phenotype 
seems  to  depend  on  the  interactions  between  the  maternal 
and  paternal  genes  (i.e.,  dominance  and  epistasis).  This 
result  is  intriguing  in  view  of  what  is  known  about  the 
ecology  of  this  species.  An  increasing  body  of  evidence 
suggests  that  competition  for  space  has  resulted  in  selec- 
tion on  growth  morphology  in  Hydractinia  species. 
Briefly,  when  two  or  more  colonies  of  the  same  size  encrust 
the  same  substratum,  the  colony  with  the  greater  periph- 
eral stolon  development  will  predominate  (Buss  et  til., 
1984;  Yund  et  ai,  1987;  Buss  and  Grosberg,  1990).  be- 
cause peripheral  stolons  are  capable  of  differentiating  into 
a  specialized  aggressive  organ,  the  hyperplastic  stolon 
(Buss  et  ai,  1984;  Lange  et  a!..  1989).  Further,  such  com- 
petition is  common  in  nature  (Buss  and  Yund,  1988; 
Yund  and  Parker,  1989),  and  geographic  variation  in 
growth  morphology  correlates  with  the  frequency  of  com- 
petition (Yund,  1987).  Nevertheless,  while  this  evidence 
suggests  that  natural  selection  favors  colonies  with  exten- 
sive peripheral  stolon  development,  colonies  with  little 
peripheral  stolon  development  are  present  in  the  popu- 
lation of  Hydractinia  symbiolongicarpus  sampled  (see 
shape  measures  in  Table  I,  and  see  McFadden  et  ai,  1984) 
and  in  other  Hydractinia  populations  (Yund,  1987). 

The  results  of  the  breeding  experiment  indicate  a  pos- 
sible explanation  for  the  maintenance  of  morphological 
variation  in  Hydractinia  populations.  While  limits  to  the 
effects  of  natural  selection  are  usually  caused  by  counter- 


vailing selection,  rather  than  exhaustion  of  additive  genetic 
variance  (see  Lande,  1988),  the  latter  has  been  implicated 
in  a  number  of  studies  (e.g.,  see  Falconer,  1981;  Lynch 
and  Sulzbach,  1984;  Hilbish  and  Koehn,  1985;  Berven, 
1987;  Travis  et  ai.  1987;  Emerson  et  ai,  1988;  Gibbs, 
1988).  Possibly,  this  has  occurred  in  Hydractinia  popu- 
lations, i.e.,  natural  selection  has  removed  much  of  the 
additive  genetic  variation  controlling  peripheral  stolon 
development,  and  what  remains  may  be  largely  non-ad- 
ditive (i.e.,  subject  to  epistatic  and  dominance  effects)  and 
thus  masked  from  selection.  We  suggest  this  only  as  a 
possibility  for  directing  future  work;  the  small  size  of  the 
laboratory  population  of  Hydractinia  precludes  any  firm 
generalization  to  natural  populations. 

Heterochrony  in  hydractiniid  hydroids 

Using  morphological  criteria,  Podocoryne  and  Hy- 
dractinia have  been  grouped  in  the  same  family  (e.g., 
Mills.  1976),  and  mtDNA  sequence  data  strongly  support 
this  interpretation  (C.  Cunningham,  pers.  comm.).  In  this 
phylogenetic  context,  variation  between  Podocoryne  and 
Hydractinia  can  be  described  in  terms  of  general  patterns 
ofheterochrony  (Gould,  1977).  As  Gould  points  out,  cer- 
tain morphological  traits  often  correlate  with  suites  of  life 
history  characteristics.  This  seems  to  be  the  case  with  Po- 
docoryne and  Hydractinia.  Relative  to  the  latter,  Podo- 
coryne  grow  and  mature  rapidly  (see  Hauenschild,  1956; 
Braverman,  1963),  produce  energetically  inexpensive 
medusae  (cf.  Schierwater,  1989;  Schierwater  and  Hauen- 
schild, 1990),  and  disperse  widely  to  new  and  varied  sub- 
strata (Podocoryne  have  swimming  medusae  and  larvae, 
while  Hydractinia  lack  medusae  and  have  crawling  lar- 
vae). Further,  the  morphology  of  Podocoryne  can  be  re- 
garded as  juvenilized  relative  to  Hydractinia.  When  either 
hydroid  fully  covers  the  substratum,  stolons  form  densely 
packed  structures  (a  fused  stolonal  mat  in  Hydractinia, 
and  a  structure  resembling  a  stolonal  mat  in  Podocoryne). 
Such  a  structure  is  thus  the  final  developmental  stage  in 
both  taxa.  Hydractinia,  however,  always  forms  some  sto- 
lonal mat  early  in  its  colony  development,  and  in  many 
cases  much  of  the  young  colony  consists  of  stolonal  mat 
(hence  the  nearly  circular  shapes  of  some  Hydractinia 
strains).  Very  young  (and  sexually  immature)  Hydractinia 
thus  attain  a  developmental  stage  (i.e..  fused  stolonal  mat 
and  nearly  circular  shape)  that  is  only  approached  by  fully 
mature  Podocoryne.  The  latter  can  thus  be  regarded  as 
paedomorphic  or  the  former  peramorphic  (see  Alberch 
etal,  1979). 

Shape  variation  in  clonal  organisms 

Many  clonal  plants,  fungi,  and  invertebrate  animals 
are  composed  of  clonally  iterated  food-gathering  units  (i.e.. 


404 


N.  W.  BLACKSTONE  AND  L.  W.  BUSS 


ramets)  connected  by  vascular  canals  ( £'.#..  stolons  or  rhi- 
zomes) that  adhere  to  the  substratum  (Boardman  el  ai, 
1973:  Larwood  and  Rosen,  1979;  Jackson  cl  at.,  1985; 
Harper  etai,  1986).  Clonal  morphologies  of  this  sort  vary 
markedly  in  the  development  of  peripheral  stolons  or  rhi- 
zomes (Buss,  1979;  Jackson,  1979;  Lovett-Doust,  1981; 
Harper,  1985).  The  approach  used  here  to  measure  vari- 
ation in  peripheral  stolon  development  in  hydractiniid 
hydroids  may  prove  useful  in  other  analyses  of  clonal 
form.  Simple  shape  measures  such  as  perimete r/\area can 
easily  be  acquired  from  properly  lighted  specimens  with 
a  simple  pixel  gradient  detector  (provided  by  most  com- 
mercially available  image  analysis  software).  Use  of  such 
characters  may  have  considerable  technical  advantages 
for  the  analysis  of  ontogenetic  and  phylogenetic  changes 
in  colony  form. 

Acknowledgments 

The  Peabody  Museum  Morphometrics  Laboratory,  the 
Social  Sciences  Statistics  Laboratory,  and  the  Yale  Com- 
puter Center  were  used  for  image  analysis  and  data  anal- 
ysis. Comments  were  provided  by  M.  Dick,  R.  Lange,  B. 
Schierwater,  and  two  reviewers.  The  National  Science 
Foundation  (BSR-88-05961)  provided  support. 

Literature  Cited 

Albcrch  P.,  S.  J.  Gould,  G.  F.  Osier,  and  D.  B.  Wake.  1979.  Size  and 
shape  in  ontogeny  and  phylogeny.  Paleobiology  5:  296-317. 

Berven,  K.  A.  1987.  The  heritable  basis  of  variation  in  larval  devel- 
opmental patterns  within  populations  of  the  wood  frog  (Rana  syl- 
valica).  Evolutional:  1088-1097. 

Blackstone,  N.  W.  1987.  Specific  growth  rates  of  parts  in  a  hermit 
crab:  a  reductionist  approach  to  the  study  of  allometry.  J.  Zoo/.  Land. 
211:531-545. 

Blackstone,  N.  W.,  and  P.  O.  Vund.  1989.  Morphological  variation  in 
a  colonial  marine  hydroid:  a  comparison  of  size-based  and  age-based 
heterochrony.  Paleobiology  15:  1-10. 

Boardman,  R.  S.,  A.  H.  Cheetam,  and  W.  A.  Oliver.  1973.  Animal 
Colonies,  Development,  ami  Function  through  Time.  Dowden, 
Hutchinson.  and  Ross.  Stroundberg.  PA. 

Bookstein,  F.  L.  1978.  The  Measurement  of  Biological  Shape  and  Shape 
Change.  Lecture  Notes  in  Biomathematics.  No.  24.  Springer- Verlag. 
New  York. 

Braverman,  M.  1963.  Studies  of  hydroid  differentiation  II.  Colony 
growth  and  the  initiation  of  sexuality.  J.  Embryol.  Exp.  Morphol. 
11:  239-253. 

Buss,  L.  W.  1979.  Habitat  selection,  directional  growth,  and  spatial 
refuges:  why  colonial  animals  have  more  hiding  places.  Pp.  459-497 
in  Biology  and  Systematic!  of  Colonial  Organisms,  G.  Larwood  and 
B.  Rosen,  eds.  Academic  Press,  London. 

Buss,  L.  W.,  and  P.  O.  Vund.  1988.  A  comparison  of  recent  and  his- 
torical populations  of  the  colonial  hydroid  Hydractinia.  Ecology  69: 
646-654. 

Buss,  L.  W.,  and  P.  O.  Yund.  1989.  A  sibling  species  group  of  Hy- 
dractinia in  the  northeastern  United  States.  J.  Mar.  Biol.  Assoc. 
U.  K.  69:  857-874. 


Buss,  L.  W.,  and  R.  K.  Grosberg.  1990.  Morphogenetic  basis  for  phe- 
notypic  differences  in  hydroid  competitive  behavior.  Nature  343:  63- 
66. 

Buss,  L.  W.,  C.  S.  McFadden,  and  D.  R.  Keene.  1984.  Biology  of  hy- 
dractiniid hydroids.  2.  Histocompatibility  effector  system/competitive 
mechanism  mediated  by  nematocyst  discharge.  Biol.  Bull.  167:  139- 
158. 

Cunningham,  C.  W'.,  L.  W.  Buss,  and  C.  Anderson,  (in  press).  Molecular 
and  geologic  evidence  of  shared  history  between  hermit  crabs  and 
the  symbiotic  genus  Hydractinia.  Evolution. 

Edwards,  C.  1972.  The  hydroids  and  the  medusae  Podocoryne  areolala. 
P  horcalis.  and  l>  earned  .1  Mar  Biol  Assoc.  i'  K  52:  97-144. 

Emerson.  S.  B.,  J.  Travis,  and  M.  Blouin.  1988.  Evaluating  a  hypothesis 
about  heterochrony:  larval  life-history  traits  and  juvenile  hind-limb 
morphology  in  Hyla  crucifer.  Evolution  42:  68-78. 

Falconer.  D.  S.  1981.  Introduction  to  Quantitative  Genetics.  2nd  ed. 
Longman,  London. 

Person,  S.,  V.  J.  Rohlf,  and  R.  K.  koehn.  1985.  Measuring  shape  vari- 
ation of  two-dimensional  outlines.  Sysl.  Zoo/.  34:  59-68. 

Gibbs,  II.  L.  1988.  Hentability  and  selection  on  clutch  size  in  Darwin's 
medium  ground  finches  (Geospiza  fortis).  Evolution  42:  750-762. 

Gould,  S.  J.  1973.  The  shape  of  things  to  come.  Syst.  Zoo/  22:  401- 
404. 

Gould,  S.  J.  1977.  Ontogeny  and  Phytogeny  Harvard  University  Press, 
Cambridge,  MA. 

Harper,  J.  L.  1985.  Modules,  branches,  and  capture  of  resources.  Pp. 
1-33  in  Biology  and  Systematic!  of  Colonial  Organisms,  G.  Larwood 
and  B.  Rosen,  eds.  Academic  Press.  London. 

Harper,  J.  L.,  B.  R.  Rosen,  and  J.  White  (eds.).  1986.  The  Growth 
and  Form  of  Modular  Organisms.  Royal  Society  Press,  London. 

Hauenschild,  C.  1954.  Genetische  entwicklungphysiologische  Unter- 
suchungen  uber  Interse-xualitat  und  Gewebevertraglichkeit  bei  Hy- 
dractinia echinata.  Roux's  Arehiv.  fur  Enlwicklungsmech.  147:  1- 
41. 

Hauenschild,  C.  1956.  Uber  die  VererbungeinerGewebevertraglichkeit- 
Eigenschaft  bei  den  Hydroidpolypen  Hydractinia  echinata.  Z.  Na- 
liirforscli.  lib:  132-138. 

Hilbish,  T.  J.,  and  R.  K.  Koehn.  1985.  Dominance  in  physiological 
phenotypes  and  fitness  at  an  enzyme  locus.  Science  229:  52-54. 

Jackson,  J.  B.  C.  1979.  Morphological  strategies  of  sessile  animals. 
Pp.  499-555  in  Biology  and  Systematic!  of  Colonial  Organisms,  G. 
Larwood  and  B.  Rosen,  eds.  Academic  Press,  London. 

Jackson,  J.  B.  C,  L.  W.  Buss,  and  R.  E.  Cook  (eds.).  1985.  Population 
Biology  and  Evolution  of  Clonal  Organisms.  Yale  University  Press, 
New  Haven.  CT. 

Lande,  R.  1988.  Quantitative  genetics  and  evolutionary  theory.  Pp. 
7 1  -84  in  Quantitative  Genetics.  B.  S.  Weir,  E.  J.  Eisen.  M.  M.  Good- 
man, and  G.  Namkoong,  eds.  Sinauer,  Sunderland.  MA. 

Lange,  R.,  G.  Pliekert,  and  W.  A.  Muller.  1989.  Histoincompatibility 
in  a  low  invertebrate.  Hydractinia  echinata:  analysis  of  a  mechanism 
of  rejection.  J.  E.\y>.  Zoo/.  249:  284-292. 

Larwood,  G.,  and  B.  Rosen  (eds.).  1979.  Biology  and  Systematic!  of 
Colonial  Organisms.  Academic  Press.  London. 

Lohman,  G.  P.  1983.  Eigenshape  analysis  of  microfossils:  a  general 
morphometnc  procedure  for  describing  changes  in  shape.  Math.  Geol. 
15:659-672. 

Lovett-Doust,  L.  1981.  Population  dynamics  and  local  specialization 
in  a  clonal  perennial  (Ranunculus  repens).  I.  The  dynamics  of  ramets 
in  contrasting  habitats.  ./.  Ecol.  69:  743-755. 

Lynch,  C.  B.,  and  D.  S.  Sulzbach.  1984.  Quantitative  genetic  analysis 
of  temperature  regulation  in  Atits  musculus.  II.  Diallel  analysis  of 
individual  traits.  Evolution  38:  527-540. 

Mazer,  S.  J.  1987.     Parental  effects  on  seed  development  and  seed  yield 


HYDRACTINIID  HYDROID  SHAPE 


405 


in  Raphanux  raphanistrwn:  implications  for  natural  and  sexual  se- 
lection. Evolution  41:  355-371. 

McFadden,  C.  S.  1986.  Laboratory  evidence  for  a  size  refuge  in  com- 
petitive interactions  between  the  hydroids  Hydractinia  echinata 
(Flemming)  and  Podocoryne  ainiea(Sars).  Bid  Bui/.  171:  161-174. 

McFadden,  C.  S.,  M.  J.  McFarland,  and  L.  \V.  Buss.  1984.  Biology 
of  hydractiniid  hydroids.  I.  Colony  ontogeny  in  Hydractinia  cchinaia 
(Hemming).  Bio/.  Bull.  166:  54-67. 

Mills,  C.  E.  1976.  Podocoryne  se/ena.  a  new  species  of  hydroid  from 
the  Gulf  of  Mexico,  and  a  comparison  with  Hydractinia  cchinaia 
Bioi  Bull.  151:  214-224. 

Mitchell-Olds,  I.  1986.  Quantitative  genetics  of  survival  and  growth 
in  Impatiem  capensis.  Evolution  40:  107-116. 

Patton,  D.  R.  1975.  A  diversity  index  for  quantifying  habitat  "edge." 
l\'ildl.  Soc.  Bull  3:  171-173. 

Plickert,  G.,  M.  Kroiher,  and  H.  Munk.  1988.  Cell  proliferation  and 
early  differentiation  during  embryonic  development  and  metamor- 
phosis of  Hvdraciinia  echinata.  Development  103:  795-803. 

Rees,  W.  J.  1941.  On  life  history  and  developmental  stages  of  the 
medusa  Padocoryne  borealis.  J  Mar.  Biol.  Assnc.  Plymouth  25:  309- 
316. 

Schierwater,  B.  1989.  Experimented  Studien  zu  den  energetischen 
Kosten  der  Fortptlanzung  bei  sexuellem  versus  vegetativem  Modus 
an  Eleitthmu  dicholoma  (Athecata,  Hydrozoa)  und  Sly/aria  laiiislris 
(Naididae.  Oligochaeta).  Ph.D.  Thesis.  University  of  Braunschweig, 
Braunschweig,  FRO. 

Schierwater.  B.,  and  C.  Hauenschild.  1990.  The  position  and  conse- 
quences of  a  vegetative  mode  of  reproduction  in  the  life  cycle  of  a 


hydromedusa  and  an  oligochaete  worm.  Pp.  37-42  in  Advances  in 

Invertebrate  Reproduction  5.  M.  Hoshi  and  O.  Yamashita,  eds.  El- 

sevier.  Amsterdam. 
Schierwater,  B.,  B.  Piekos,  and  L.  \V.  Buss,  (in  press).     Hydroid  stolon 

contractions  mediated  by  contractile  vacuoles.  J.  E.\p.  Biol 
Sokal,  R.  R.,  and  F.  J.  Rohlf.  1981.     Biometry.  2nded.  W.  H.  Freeman, 

San  Francisco. 
Spindler,  A.  N.,  and  \V.  A.  Muller.  1972.     Induction  of  metamorphosis 

by  bacteria  and  by  lithium  pulse  in  the  larvae  of  Hydractinia  echinata 

(Hydrozoa).  Wilhelm  Roux' Arch.  Entwicklungsmech,  Org  169:271- 

280. 
Travis,  J.,  S.  B.  Emerson,  and  M.  Bluuin.  1987.     A  quantitative  genetic 

analysis  of  larval  life  history  traits  in  Ilyla  crueller.  Evolution  41: 

145-156. 

Via,  S.  1988.     Estimating  variance  components:  reply  to  Groeters.  Evo- 
lution 42:  633-634. 
Weis,  V.  M.,  and  L.  W.  Buss.  1987.     infrastructure  of  metamorphosis 

in  Hvdraciinia  cchinaia.  Poxlilla  199. 
Yund,  P.  O.  1987.     Intraspecific  competition  and  the  maintenance  of 

morphological  variation  in  the  colonial  marine  hydroid  Hydractinia 

symbioacadianus.  Ph.D.  Thesis.  Yale  LIniversity,  New  Haven.  CT. 
Yund,  P.  O.,  C.  W.  Cunningham,  and  L.  \V.  Buss.  1987.     Recruitment 

and  postrecruitment  interactions  in  a  colonial  hydroid.  Ecology  68: 

971-982. 
Yund,  P.  O.,  and  H.  M.  Parker.  1989.     Population  structure  of  the 

colonial  hydroid  Hydractinia  sp.  nov.  C  in  the  Gulf  of  Maine.  / 

Exp.  Mar.  Biol.  Ecol  125:  63-82. 


Reference:  Binl.  Bull  180:  406-415.  (June,  1991) 


Inducible  Agonistic  Structures  in  the  Tropical 
Corallimorpharian,  Discosoma  sanctithomae 

J.  S.  MILES 

Marine  Science  Center,  Northeastern  University,  Nahant,  Massachusetts  01908 


Abstract.  The  Corallimorpharia  are  a  group  of  soft- 
bodied  anthozoans  closely  related  to  the  scleractinian 
corals.  Although  numerous  reports  have  documented  the 
agonistic  behaviors  of  actiniarians  and  hard  corals,  only 
Chadwick  (1987)  has  shown  such  behaviors  in  a  coralli- 
morph  (Corynactis  California).  The  following  investigation 
confirms  the  use  of  inducible  aggressive  structures  in  space 
competition  in  the  laboratory  and  in  the  field  by  Disco- 
soma sanctithomae.  This  tropical  corallimorph  used  both 
modified  marginal  tentacles  and  mesenterial  filaments  to 
damage  adjacent  scleractinians.  All  colonies  ofAgaricia 
agaricites  transplanted  near  D.  sanctithomae  were  dam- 
aged. Initially,  D.  sanctithomae  adjacent  to  Meandrina 
meandrites  were  severely  wounded.  However,  67%  re- 
covered and  retaliated  within  a  one  to  six  month  period, 
causing  damage  to  M.  meandrina  that  persisted  for  at 
least  twelve  months. 

Introduction 

Many  benthic  cnidarians  that  reproduce  asexually  ex- 
pand their  colonies  proximally  and  radially.  New  indi- 
viduals require  space  on  the  substrate  to  become  estab- 
lished and  to  grow.  In  a  coral  reef  environment,  however, 
space  is  limiting,  and  as  an  individual,  or  clone,  expands, 
competitive  interactions  are  common.  These  competitive 
encounters  may  have  provided  important  selective  pres- 
sures for  the  evolution  of  agonistic  behaviors  and  struc- 
tures to  deal  with  these  competition  events.  Abel  (1954) 
first  described  acrorhagi,  the  inflatable  sacks  around  the 
collar  of  certain  Actiniaria.  Francis  ( 1 973a)  noticed  a  par- 
ticular spatial  pattern  among  conspecific  anemone  clones 
ofAnthopleura  elegantissima:  individuals  of  a  clone  were 
closely  aggregated,  but  groups  of  different  clones  were  al- 
ways separated  by  an  "anemone-free  zone."  This  led 

Received  17  September  1990;  accepted  24  January  1991. 


Francis  to  describe  a  series  of  behaviors  in  which  A.  ele- 
gantissima used  acrorhagi  during  agonistic  interactions 
with  non-clonemates.  Other  researchers  have  investigated 
various  aspects  of  aggressive  behavior  in  members  of  the 
class  Anthozoa  (Bonnin,  1964;  Williams,  1975;  den  Har- 
tog,  1977;  Purcell,  1977;  Bigger,  1980;  Kaplan,  1983; 
Sammarco  et  ai,  1983;  Bak  and  Borsboom,  1984;  Hidaka 
and  Yamazato,  1984;  Sebens,  1984;  Chadwick.  1987). 
Knowledge  has  evolved  from  initial  descriptions  of 
straightforward,  predictable  results  of  spatial  competition 
events  (Lang.  1971,  1973;  Francis,  1973b;  Chornesky. 
1983;  Chornesky  and  Williams,  1983)  to  descriptions  of 
more  complex,  dynamic  interactions.  The  importance  of 
temporal  scale  was  recognized,  and  many  competitive 
outcomes  were  discovered  to  be  reversible  (Bak  et  ai, 
1982;  Logan,  1984;  Chornesky,  1985).  The  initial  victor 
was  not  always  the  ultimate  winner.  Other  factors  such 
as  size,  attack  angle,  and  previous  aggressive  history  af- 
fected the  outcome  of  competitions  (Brace  and  Pavey, 
1978;  Brace,  1981;  Bak  et  al.  1982).  Also,  significant  work 
has  been  done  elucidating  the  systems  of  recognition  re- 
quired for  these  agonistic  behaviors  (Theodor,  1970;  Hil- 
demann,  1974;  Bigger,  1980;  Sauer  et  al..  1986).  The 
number  of  species  known  to  exhibit  specialized  structures 
used  in  aggressive  behaviors  has  also  increased,  including 
members  from  four  different  orders  within  the  class  An- 
thozoa (reviewed  by  Lang  and  Chornesky,  1988). 

In  addition  to  acrorhagi,  certain  species  in  the  order 
Actiniaria  employ  a  modified  feeding  tentacle  as  a  fighting 
tentacle  (=  catch  tentacles,  Purcell,  1977).  Functionally 
similar  to  the  acrorhagi,  this  elongated  tentacle  can  adhere 
to  neighboring  non-clonemate  conspecifics,  causing  tissue 
necrosis  and  ultimately,  if  successful,  retreat  of  the  op- 
position. As  with  A.  elegantissimma.  these  behaviors  have 
been  reported  to  produce  single-clone  aggregates  separated 
by  anemone-free  zones  (Purcell,  1977;  Purcell  and  Kitting, 
1982).  One  report  indicated  the  mechanism  also  worked 


406 


CORALLIMORPHARIAN  AGGRESSION 


407 


on  an  intrasexual  level,  yielding  anemone-free  zones  be- 
tween clones  of  the  same  sex  (Kaplan,  1983).  Some  scler- 
actmians  possess  a  structure  similar  to  the  actiniarians' 
fighting  tentacle.  Sweeper  tentacles,  so  termed  because 
they  sweep  the  adjacent  area,  develop  on  polyps  of  certain 
reef  corals  (Richardson  et  at.,  1979;  Chornesky,  1983; 
Chornesky  and  Williams,  1983;  Hidaka  and  Yamazato. 
1984).  The  development  of  sweeper  tentacles  is  induced 
by  the  presence  of,  or  aggression  by,  another  coral  (Chor- 
nesky, 1983).  These  interactions  are  primarily  interspecific 
and  are  often  used  in  conjunction  with  a  second  mech- 
anism. Lang(1971,  1973)and  Logan  (1984)  described  the 
process  of  extracoelenteric  digestion  used  by  reef  corals 
to  avoid  being  overgrown  and  to  acquire  new  space.  The 
extrusion  of  mesenterial  filaments  through  the  mouth  and 
body  wall  onto  another  coral  results  in  partial  mortality 
of  the  opposing  colony. 

Although  the  Octocorallia  use  allelochemicals  in  com- 
petitive interactions  (Sammarco  et  a/.,  1983,  1985; 
LaBarre,  1986;  Pawlik  et  ai.  1987),  until  recently  no 
member  of  this  subclass  was  reported  to  have  specialized 
structures  used  for  aggression.  Several  reports  described 
sweeper-like  tentacles  on  species  of  Alcyonacea  and  Gor- 
gonacea,  but  these  tentacles  are  probably  feeding  apparati 
(Abel,  1970;  Muzik,  1983).  However,  Erythropodium 
caribaeorum  (Gorgonacea),  develops  sweeper  tentacles 
and  uses  them  for  aggression  (Sebens  and  Miles,  1988). 
These  structures  function  in  the  same  way  as  the  sweeper 
tentacles  of  the  scleractinians,  but  instead  of  only  one  or 
two  tentacles  per  polyp  becoming  sweepers,  all  eight  of 
the  tentacles  on  many  polyps  elongate  and  are  able  to 
damage  neighboring  corals.  It  is  of  interest  to  note  that 
E.  caribaeorum  is  the  only  obligate  encrusting  gorgonian 
in  the  Caribbean.  This  growth  form  inevitably  leads  to 
interactions  with  a  variety  of  other  species  requiring  space 
on  the  primary  substratum. 

The  Corallimorpharia  are  another  order  with  members 
exhibiting  agonistic  behaviors.  These  soft-bodied  members 
of  the  Hexacorallia  resemble  anemones,  but  are  related 
more  closely,  morphologically  and  phylogenetically.  to 
the  scleractinians.  Chadwick  ( 1987)  reported  that  the  cor- 
allimorph  Corynactis  califarnica  used  mesenterial  fila- 
ments against  species  of  anemones  and  corals  in  agonistic 
interactions  in  the  laboratory.  Earlier,  den  Hartog  (1977) 
described  two  types  of  tentacles  along  the  rim  of  the  oral 
disk  of  the  corallimorph  Discosoma  sanctithomae  (Du- 
chassaing  and  Michelotti).  Some  of  these  marginal  ten- 
tacles are  thin  and  hair-like,  whereas  others  are  finger- 
like  and  bulbous.  He  found  these  bulbous  tentacles  to 
have  larger  and  more  dense  holotrichous  nematocysts  than 
the  thin  counterparts  and  suggested  that  these  might  rep- 
resent a  morphological  variant  used  in  agonistic  encoun- 
ters, although  no  experimental  work  was  done.  Sebens 


(1976)  examined  this  species  in  field  and  laboratory  studies 
in  Panama  but  found  no  evidence  of  agonistic  behavior 
in  short-term  experiments.  Here,  I  report  the  first  field 
results  demonstrating  agonistic  behavior  in  competitions 
between  a  corallimorph  and  several  species  of  scleractinian 
corals.  D.  sanctithomae  used  both  mesenterial  filaments 
and  enlarged  marginal  tentacles  to  damage  the  scleractin- 
ian corals  Agaricia  agaricites  and  Meandrina  meandrites. 

Materials  and  Methods 

Site  location  and  description 

Field  and  laboratory  experiments  were  completed  at 
the  Discovery  Bay  Marine  Laboratory  in  Discovery  Bay. 
Jamaica  (18°30'N:  77°20'W).  The  reef  crest  along  the 
north  coast  of  Jamaica  runs  predominantly  east  to  west, 
with  spur  and  groove  formations  jutting  out  to  the  north. 
At  Discovery  Bay,  the  fore  reef  is  separated  from  a  well- 
developed  lagoon  by  a  conspicuous  reef  crest  that  has 
mounds  of  exposed  coral  rubble  accumulated  from  Hur- 
ricane Allen  in  1980.  Spur  and  groove  formations  begin 
at  10m  on  the  fore  reef  and  continue  to  the  fore  reef 
slope,  which  occurs  at  2 1  m  at  some  locations.  The  east 
back  reef  is  predominantly  a  sandy  bottom  with  Thalassia 
beds  interspersed  with  patch  reefs.  Columbus  Park  is  an 
area  of  the  back  reef  with  high  concentrations  of  silt  and 
of  paniculate  matter  that  reduces  visibility.  Shallow  areas 
are  dominated  by  benthic  soft-bodied  zoantharians  within 
a  dead  Acropora  cervicornis  framework.  The  deeper  re- 
gions possess  a  rich  sponge  and  mollusk  community.  For 
a  more  detailed  description,  see  Goreau  (1959),  Goreau 
and  Goreau  (1973),  and  Liddell  et  ai.  (1984).  Survey  data 
were  collected  from  the  west  back  reef  and  Columbus 
Park,  and  from  Long  Term  Study  (LTS),  Kinzie's  Reef, 
and  Lynton's  Mine  on  the  fore  reef.  Transplant  experi- 
ments were  located  at  all  the  fore  reef  survey  sites  between 
depths  of  10  m  and  20  m  (Fig.  1 ). 

Animal  descriptions  ami  collection 

Discosoma  sanctithomae  is  a  corallimorpharian  com- 
mon throughout  the  Caribbean  and  Bermuda  between 
depths  of  1-20  m  (den  Hartog,  1980).  Both  solitary  in- 
dividuals and  asexually  produced  clonal  aggregates  can 
be  found  living  within  the  coral  reef  framework.  The  an- 
imal is  orally-aborally  flattened,  with  the  oral  disk  aver- 
aging approximately  4  cm  in  diameter  (Fig.  2A).  A  margin 
at  the  edge  of  the  oral  disk  lacks  tentacles  and  is  often 
tucked  up  under  the  disk,  but  at  other  times  is  expanded 
well  beyond  the  basal  attachment  area.  The  oral  disk  ten- 
tacles of  D.  sanctithomae  are  very  short,  stubby,  and  are 
often  ramous.  A  second  group  of  tentacles  extends  radially 
from  the  margin  of  the  disk  and  are  thus  termed  marginal 
tentacles. 


40S 


J.  S.  MILES 


T\ 


Figure  1.     Map  of  the  fore  reef  at  Discovery  Bay.  Jamaica.  Areas  marked  with  stars  indicate  sites  of 
transplant  experiments  and  tore  reef  survey  sites. 


Specimens  ofDiscosoma  sanctithomae  used  in  the  field 
experiments  were  never  removed  from  the  reef  or  dis- 
turbed in  any  way.  Individuals  were  identified  by  tags 
placed  on  nearby  coral  rubble.  D.  sanctithomae  used  in 
laboratory  experiments  were  collected  along  with  pieces 
of  the  substrate,  usually  dead  Acropora  cervicornis,  because 
attempts  to  scrape  off  individuals  were  always  unsuccess- 
ful. The  collected  animals  were  placed  in  a  running  sea- 
water  table  and  allowed  to  acclimate  for  three  to  seven 
days  before  being  used  in  experiments. 

The  sderactinian  corals  Agaricia  agaricites  (Pallas)  and 
Meandrina  meandrites  (L)  were  used  in  the  field  trans- 
plants. Pieces  of  coral  (approximately  6X8  cm)  were 
collected  using  a  rock  hammer  and  chisel  to  release  them 
at  their  base  or  at  an  area  of  dead  coral  skeleton.  The 
corals  were  collected  from  the  same  reefs  onto  which  they 
were  to  be  transplanted.  They  were  not  brought  to  the 
surface,  but  were  left  for  two  to  seven  days  before  being 
transplanted.  Corals  were  not  used  if  they  showed  signs 
of  tissue  damage  from  the  collection  methods  within  this 
period.  A.  agaricites  and  M.  meandrites  used  in  the  lab- 


oratory experiments  were  collected  in  a  similar  manner. 
However,  these  corals  were  transferred  to  running  sea- 
water  tables  and  allowed  to  acclimate  for  three  to  seven 
days. 

Field  surveys 

Field  surveys  of  Discosoma  sanctithomae  were  con- 
ducted to  determine  which  organisms  lived  adjacent  to 
the  corallimorph  and  what  interactions  were  occurring. 
The  surveys  were  done  by  swimming  parallel  transects 
across  depth  contours  throughout  a  designated  area  and 
recording  every  D.  sanctithomae  observed.  Each  of  the  D. 
sanctithomae' s  neighbors  were  noted,  and  any  damage  on 
either  D.  sanctithomae  or  any  neighbor  was  recorded. 
Percent-cover  data  were  gathered  from  the  same  area  as 
the  D.  sanctithomae  survey.  Survey  procedures  involved 
assigning  random  numbers  to  a  chain-link  transect  that 
was  haphazardly  dropped  within  the  study  area.  Species 
or  substrate  type  that  fell  under  each  of  the  marked  chain 
links  was  recorded  (Rogers  et  a/.,  1983).  Information  from 


CORAl.LIMORPHARIAN  AGGRESSION 


409 


Figure  2.  (A)  Agarkia  agarkiic.i  (a)  transplanted  next  to  a  Discnsoma  sanclithomae  with  thin  marginal 
tentacles  (arrows).  Scale  =  1  cm.  (B)  D  sanctithomae  adjacent  to  Meandrina  meandrites  (M)  developed 
large  swollen  acrospheres  (a)  at  the  tips  of  the  marginal  tentacles.  Areas  of  the  rim  have  become  enlarged 
also.  Scale  ~  2  mm.  (C)  D  sancnihnmac  with  acrospheres.  Mesenterial  filaments  (mf)  can  be  seen  in  the 
area  of  the  coelenteron  leading  to  the  marginal  tentacles.  Scale  =  2  mm 


the  D.  sanctithomae  survey  and  the  percent  cover  transects 
was  compared  to  determine  whether  D.  sanclithomae" s 
contact  with  neighbors  was  random  or  reflected  some  sort 
of  selection  for,  or  by,  neighboring  species. 

Transplant  experiments 

Field.  Manipulative  experiments  were  done  in  the  field 
to  study  the  interactions  ofDiscosoma  sanctithomae  and 
scleractinian  corals.  Pieces  of  coral  were  epoxied  adjacent 
to,  but  not  touching,  individual  D.  sanctithomae,  which 
were  partially  retracted.  However,  transplants  were  located 
so  that  fully  expanded  D.  sanctithomae  would  touch  the 
coral's  tissue.  The  Pettit  Underwater  Patching  Compound 
used  to  fix  the  corals  in  place  was  not  toxic  when  applied 
only  to  the  dead  base  of  the  coral.  Corals  showing  a  general 
tissue  necrosis  (possibly  from  handling)  soon  after  the 


transplant  ( 1-2  days)  were  removed  from  the  study;  this 
accounts  for  most  of  the  discrepencies  between  initial  and 
final  sample  sizes.  Specific  sets  of  transplants  were  de- 
signed to  address  the  following  questions:  (1)  Can  D. 
sanctithomae  damage  scleractinian  corals  adjacent  to 
them?  (2)  Are  bulbous  marginal  tentacles  with  acrospheres 
associated  with  the  damage  to  corals?  (3)  Do  D.  sanctitho- 
mae react  differently  depending  on  the  species  of  coral 
that  is  next  to  them? 

Three  sets  of  transplants  (Series  I)  were  begun  in  Jan- 
uary 1987.  In  the  first  set  of  transplants  (Tl)  pieces  of 
Agaricia  agari cites  (n  =  19)  were  placed  next  to  D.  sanc- 
tithomaev/ith  filiform  marginal  tentacles  (Fig.  2A).  A  sec- 
ond group  of  transplants  (T2)  paired  A.  agaricites  (n  =  16) 
with  D.  sanctithomae  that  had  bulbous  marginal  tentacles. 
A.  agaricites  was  chosen  because  it  was  found  frequently 
next  to  D.  sanctithomae  in  the  field  surveys.  Both  sets  of 


410 


J.  S.  MILES 


transplants  were  designed  to  examine  D.  sanctithomae's 
tendency  to  damage  corals.  Also,  if  the  bulbous  tentacles 
were  responsible  for  damage  to  the  scleractinians,  the  cor- 
als in  T2  would  be  expected  to  incur  damage  more  quickly 
than  the  corals  in  Tl.  Meandrina  meandrites  (n  =  18) 
was  used  in  a  third  transplant  experiment  (T3)  to  test  for 
any  variation  in  response  by  D.  sanctithomae.  Unlike  A. 
agaricites,  this  coral  is  known  to  use  mesenterial  filaments 
readily  in  aggressive  encounters  (Lang,  1973;  Logan, 
1 984).  All  experimental  pairs  were  monitored  for  damage 
to  D.  sanctithomae  or  to  A.  agaricites,  and  for  any  changes 
in  the  morphology  of  the  marginal  tentacles  on  D.  sanc- 
tithomae once  a  week  for  five  weeks,  again  after  five 
months,  then  after  one  year.  Several  night  dives  were  done 
to  confirm  damage  to  coral  polyps  and  to  check  for  de- 
velopment of  sweeper  tentacles  (Chornesky,  1983).  Pho- 
tographs of  experimental  pairs  were  taken  weekly  with  a 
Nikonos  camera  with  a  2: 1  extension  tube,  and  a  Minolta 
XL40 1  Super-8  movie  camera  was  left  on  the  reef  for  four 
days  at  a  time,  taking  photographs  at  1.5-min  intervals. 
A  second  series  of  transplants  (Series  II)  was  started  in 
February  1988.  These  experiments  were  identical  to  the 
1987  Tl  and  T2  transplants  except  that  they  were  mon- 
itored once  a  day  for  two  and  a  half  weeks  to  examine 
the  interactions  over  a  shorter  time  period. 

Two  types  of  controls  were  used  to  test  for  the  effects 
of  the  transplantation  process.  Pieces  of  coral  transplanted 
near  Discosoma  sanctithomae  were  always  large  enough 
so  that  at  least  half  of  their  tissue  area  was  out  of  reach 
of  the  D.  sanctithomae.  even  when  fully  expanded  (op- 
posite-side controls).  Additional  pieces  of  coral  were 
transplanted  among  the  experimental  pairs,  but  not  within 
reach  of  any  D.  sanctithomae.  These  corals  were  regularly 
examined  for  any  signs  of  damage.  A  control  for  D.  sanc- 
tithomae acrosphere  formation  was  done  by  observing 
the  marginal  tentacles  of  two  sets  of  D.  sanctithomae  that 
were  surrounded  only  by  algae.  One  group  of  D.  sanc- 
tithomae was  monitored  once  every  ten  days  for  two 
months.  The  second  group  was  monitored  every  day  for 
up  to  twenty  days.  These  individuals  were  studied  to  de- 
termine whether  sporadic  changes  in  the  marginal  ten- 
tacles occurred  without  contact  with  cnidarian  neighbors. 

Laboratory  experiments.  Transplant  experiments  sim- 
ilar to  those  in  the  field  were  done  in  the  laboratory  in 
running  seawater  tables.  Discosoma  sanctithomae  indi- 
viduals were  paired  with  pieces  ofAgaricia  agaricites  (n 
=  5)  and  Meandrina  meandrites  (n  =  5).  None  of  the  D. 
sanctithomae  had  enlarged  marginal  tentacles,  nor  did 
the  corals  have  sweeper  tentacles  at  the  beginning  of  the 
experiment.  When  both  members  of  an  experimental  pair 
were  contracted,  neither  touched  the  other.  Three  pieces 
of  A.  agaricites  and  two  of  M.  meandrites.  as  well  as  three 
individuals  of  D.  sanctithomae.  were  out  of  reach  of  any 
other  anthozoan  and  acted  as  the  controls.  These  pairs 


were  inspected  for  damage  every  hour  for  the  first  seven 
hours.  Throughout  the  experiment  a  Super-8  movie  cam- 
era with  an  intervalometer  photographed  individual  pairs 
every  1-5  min.  The  experiment  continued  for  eight  days. 


Results 


Survcv  data 


The  neighboring  species  of  more  than  155  Discosoma 
sanctithomae  were  recorded  (Fig.  3A,  B).  Approximately 
37%  (n  =  238)  of  them  were  foliose  or  turf  complex  algae, 
the  largest  group  total.  The  second  most  common  group 
found  adjacent  to  D.  sanctithomae  were  crustose  coralline 


300 


200 


100 


E 

3 


r_]     Neighbors  without  damage 
B     Neighbors  with  damage 


dl 


V 

on 


,0 


Percent  Cover 

Percent  ot  Neighboring  Species 


Figure  3.  (Top)  The  area  adjacent  to  individual  Discosoma  sanc- 
lillinmac  were  surveyed  at  all  transplant  sites  on  the  tore  reef  and  in 
Columbus  Park.  Neighboring  species  and  substrate  type  were  counted 
and  classified  as  damaged  or  not  damaged;  exact  counts  are  reported 
above  bars.  Each  organism  and  substrate  type  counted  as  "  1 "  interaction 
regardless  of  size:  this  may  underestimate  the  impact  of  larger  organisms 
and  overestimate  those  of  smaller  ones.  (Bottom)  Surveys  of  species  per- 
cent-cover were  done  at  all  the  neighbor  survey  sites.  Sixteen  transects 
were  completed:  713  chain-link  transect  points  were  classified  into  the 
same  eight  catagories  used  for  the  neighbor  survey.  Numbers  above  each 
bar  represent  percentages  for  each  group.  Percent-cover  results  (black) 
are  compared  to  percentage  data  of  neighboring  species  (white:  n  =  64 1 ). 


CORALLIMORPHARIAN  AGGRESSION 


41 


algae  (20.4%),  followed  by  the  scleractinian  corals  (18%). 
The  corals  found  most  frequently  adjacent  to  the  coral- 
limorphs  were  Alontastrea  annularis.  Sidentstreu  siderea, 
and  Agarida  agaricites.  In  more  than  75%  of  the  cases 
when  D.  sanctithomae  was  adjacent  to  a  scleractinian, 
there  were  areas  of  dead  coral  associated  with  the  area  of 
contact.  Damage  was  not  readily  apparent  in  any  other 
group  (Fig.  3A). 

Although  algae  were  also  the  most  abundant  organisms 
in  the  surveys  of  percent  cover  (n  =  557,  78%),  sclerac- 
tinian corals  (9%-)  and  sand  (8%)  were  the  second  and 
third  most  commonly  occurring  items,  respectively  (Fig. 
3B).  The  complement  of  species  and  groups  neighboring 
D.  sanctithomae  proved  to  be  significantly  different  than 
the  proportion  of  species  expected  from  the  percent  cover 
survey  using  a  G-test  for  independence  (G  =  21.03,  P 
<0.05).  Algae  have  been  dominant  space  occupiers  in 
the  fore  reef  community  since  the  die-off  of  Diadcnui  an- 
tillamm  in  1983  (Liddell  and  Ohlhorst,  1983). 

Field  transplant '.v 

Series  I.  Sixteen  of  seventeen  Discosoma  sanctithomae 
originally  with  filiform  tentacles  had  developed  bulbous 
tentacles  with  acrospheres  in  the  presence  of  Aguricia 
agaricites  within  six  weeks.  The  mean  time  for  acrosphere 
development  was  17.3  ±  2.0  days  (mean  ±  S.E.).  During 
this  time,  all  17  of  the  A.  agaricites  colonies  had  been 
damaged;  the  mean  time  to  damage  from  each  colony 
was  \1.9  ±  1 .8  days  (mean  ±  S.E.)  (Fig.  4A).  In  compar- 
ison, only  one  opposite-side  control  was  damaged.  For- 
mation of  acrospheres  and  the  occurrence  of  damage  was 
significantly  greater  than  that  which  might  occur  by 
chance  (G-test  with  William's  correction  factor:  G  =  15.5; 
G  =  23.0  resp.;  P  <  0.05).  The  time  to  acrosphere  devel- 
opment was  not  significantly  different  from  the  average 
time  for  damage  to  occur  to  the  corals  (Mann- Whitney; 
U'=  170.5,  P<  0.05). 

All  12  Agaricia  agaricites  colonies  placed  next  to  the 
Discosoma  sanctithomae  that  had  acrospheres  at  the  start 
of  the  experiment  (T2)  were  damaged.  The  average  time 
to  damage  ( 10.0  ±  1 .7  days,  mean  ±  S.E.)  was  significantly 
faster  than  the  time  to  damage  for  the  transplants  that 
later  formed  acrospheres  (Tl )  (Mann- Whitney:  U  =  204, 
P  <  0.05)  (Fig.  4 A).  Again,  only  one  opposite-side  coral 
control  was  damaged.  Sweeper  tentacles  on  A.  agaricites 
occurred  on  only  one  colony.  They  formed  after  the  in- 
teractions had  progressed  for  about  one  month  and  after 
the  coral  had  been  initially  damaged  by  the  corallimorph. 
The  corresponding  D.  sanctithomae  did  not  reveal  any 
damage. 

The  results  of  the  Discosoma  sanctithomae  transplants 
with  Meandhna  meandrites  were  strikingly  different. 
Within  one  week,  38%  of  the  D.  sanctithomae  (n  =  16) 


.e 
= 

3 


T1 :  A  (n=l7)         animals  w.'  acrospherea 

Tl    n  in-17)          corals  w  damage 
TZ:  D  (n=1 2)          corals  w/  damags 


10 


20  30 

Time  (Days) 


4H 


50 


ioo  r 


XII 


C         60 


40 


:o 


Damage  lo  Discosoma 
Damage  lo  Meandhna 


11 


2') 


ISO 


Time  (Days) 


Figure  4.  (Top)  Results  of  two  field  transplant  experiments  (Tl  & 
T2)  are  shown.  In  the  first  transplant  (Tl )  17  corals  were  placed  next  to 
Disctmima  sancnthonuic  with  thin  marginal  tentacles.  T1:A  (A)  charts 
the  progression  of  acrosphere  development  in  D.  sanctithomae.  T1:D 
(O)  tracks  the  development  of  damage  to  the  corals.  In  T2,  12  corals 
were  next  to  D.  saiMithomae  that  possessed  acrospheres.  T2:D  (•)  records 
the  damage  incurred  to  the  adjacent  corals.  (Bottom)  Results  of  field 
transplant  3  (T3).  Eighteen  Meandrina  meandrites  were  placed  near  D 
sanctithomae  with  acrospheres.  Initially  38%  of  the  D.  sanctithomae  were 
severely  injured:  two  died  within  the  first  month.  Although  none  of  the 
corals  suffered  damage  before  the  end  of  the  first  month,  67%  of  the 
remaining  transplants  were  damaged  over  the  subsequent  six-month  pe- 
riod and  remained  damaged  for  at  least  twelve  months. 


had  suffered  severe  body  lesions  from  the  mesenterial  fil- 
aments of  M.  meandrites  (Fig.  4B).  This  increased  to  63% 
within  two  weeks,  culminating  in  the  death  of  two  D. 
sanctithomae  individuals  within  the  first  month.  The  first 
incidence  of  damage  to  M.  meandrites  did  not  occur  until 
almost  one  month  had  passed.  However,  of  those  D.  sanc- 
tithomae that  survived  the  first  two  months  (n  =  12),  67% 
went  on  to  damage  the  M.  meandrites  over  the  next  four 
months.  Damage  inflicted  by  these  D.  sanctithomae  was 
still  visible  twelve  months  later  (Fig.  5).  None  of  the  op- 
posite-side coral  controls  were  damaged. 

Series  II.  Results  of  the  Series  II  transplants  were  similar 
to  Series  I  results.  Ten  of  eleven  A.  agaricites  corals  placed 


412 


J.  S.  MILES 


Figure  5.  DiM-uwniu  sanctithomae  vs.  Mcandnna  meandrites:  in- 
teraction after  twelve  months.  Note  the  large  acrospheres  (a)  and  the 
algae-covered  dead  coral  skeleton  (d).  Live  coral  polyps  (p)  can  be  seen 
out  of  reach  of  the  D.  sanctithomae's  tentacles.  Scale  =  2.7  mm. 


next  to  Discosoma  sanctithomae  with  acrospheres  were 
damaged  in  12.0  ±  1.5  days  (mean  ±  S.E.).  Only  two  of 
the  eleven  corals  adjacent  to  D.  sanctithomae  without  ac- 
rospheres were  clearly  damaged  (6.0  ±  5.0  days,  mean 
±  S.E.).  Only  three  of  eleven  had  developed  acrospheres 
within  the  trial  period  of  10  to  20  days.  This  short  period 
before  damage  occurred  reflects  one  coral  that  was  exten- 
sively damaged  after  the  second  day  by  mesenterial  fila- 
ments that  were  released  through  the  marginal  tentacles 
of  the  corallimorph.  Mesenterial  filaments  were  observed 
being  extruded  by  D.  sanctithomae  through  the  tentacles 
on  numerous  other  occasions,  and  were  extruded  fre- 
quently from  the  mouth  and  through  the  body  wall  as 
well. 

Controls 

None  of  the  isolated  coral  controls  experienced  any 
tissue  damage.  The  lack  of  incidences  of  damage  to  op- 
posite-side coral  controls  were  reported  with  the  results 
for  the  particular  transplant.  The  D.  sanctithomae  indi- 
viduals acting  as  controls  for  random  acrosphere  devel- 
opment showed  little  change;  no  acrospheres  were  formed. 
There  were,  however,  frequent  influxes  and  effluxes  of 
mesenterial  filaments  to  and  from  the  marginal  tentacles. 
At  times  the  filaments  remained  between  the  mesenteries 
in  the  coelenteron,  and  at  other  times  they  traveled  into 
the  tips  of  the  marginal  tentacles. 

Laboratory  experiments 

The  results  of  the  laboratory  experiments  were  a  brief 
accelerated  version  of  the  field  experiments.  All  the  corals, 
and  the  Discosoma  sanctithomae  individuals,  retracted 


their  polyps  when  the  experimental  pairs  were  initially 
established.  Although  eight  of  ten  corals  partially  ex- 
panded their  polyps  within  20  min,  and  all  the  D.  sanc- 
tithomae adjacent  to  A.  agaricites  had  relaxed  within  the 
first  hour,  there  was  no  direct  contact.  The  D.  sanctitho- 
mae next  to  the  M.  meandrites  remained  contracted,  with 
four  of  them  extruding  mesenterial  filaments  within  the 
first  seven  hours. 

Meandrina  meandrites  transplants  were  equally  active. 
All  five  corals  released  mesenterial  filaments  onto  the 
Discosoma  sanctithomae  within  the  first  7  h.  D.  sancti- 
thomae near  A  agaricites  also  extruded  filaments  but,  in 
general,  the  severity  of  such  attacks  was  greatly  reduced 
compared  to  those  with  M.  meandrites.  Within  24  h,  4 
D.  sanctithomae  had  mucus  layers  covering  body  wounds 
inflicted  by  M.  meandrites'  mesenterial  filaments.  Body 
postures  were  strongly  evasive,  especially  when  compared 
to  D.  sanctithomae  near  A  agaricites:  the  latter  often  laid 
their  marginal  disks  over  the  coral  surfaces.  By  the  end 
of  the  third  day,  four  of  the  five  corallimorphs  near  M. 
meandrites  were  dead.  The  fifth  had  severe  body  lesions 
and  had  partially  released  its  hold  on  the  substrate.  None 
of  the  M.  meandrites  individuals  were  damaged. 

None  of  the  Agaricia  agaricites  individuals  were  ob- 
served releasing  mesenterial  filaments,  nor  using  sweeper 
tentacles.  A  general  pattern  evolved  for  the  D.  sanctitho- 
mae-A.  agaricites  interactions  of  gradual  expansion  of  the 
D.  sanctithomae  onto  the  coral's  surface  followed  by  re- 
traction, and  intermittent  extrusion  of  mesenterial  fila- 
ments by  D.  sanctithomae.  Discosoma  sanctithomae  was 
able  to  damage  A.  agaricites  in  two  separate  cases,  al- 
though the  coral  recovered  its  damaged  area  in  one  of 
these  events.  Three  D.  sanctithomae  adjacent  to  A.  agar- 
icites died  within  eight  days,  but  two  of  these  deaths  must 
be  qualified.  Two  of  the  fatalities  resulted  from  the  D. 
sanctithomae  releasing  its  hold  near  the  A.  agaricites  and 
wandering  into  a  colony  of  M.  meandrites.  None  of  the 
D.  sanctithomae  individuals  formed  marginal  tentacles. 
All  the  control  corals  survived  without  damage,  and  one 
of  the  three  control  D.  sanctithomae  individuals  died. 

Discussion 

Although  lacking  a  hard  skeleton,  the  soft-bodied  rel- 
atives of  the  scleractinians,  such  as  the  Actiniaria,  have 
proven  to  be  able  competitors  for  space  (Francis,  1973a; 
Purcell,  1 977;  Purcell  and  Kitting,  1982;Chadwick.  1987). 
More  recently,  investigators  have  discovered  that  the 
Corallimorpharia  possess  aggressive  abilities  as  well.  Dur- 
ing prolonged  interspecific  exposure,  Corynactis  califor- 
nica  killed  polyps  of  the  actiniarians  Anthopleura  elegan- 
tissima  and  Metridium  senile,  as  well  as  the  scleractinians 
Astrangia  lajo/laensis  and  Balanophyllia  elegans  by  ex- 
truding mesenterial  filaments  (Chadwick,  1987).  The 


CORAI  LIMORPHARIAN  AGGRESSION 


413 


present  study  supplies  evidence  that  another  corallimorph, 
Discosoma  sanctithomae,  can  compete  successfully  with 
scleractinian  corals  for  primary  space.  Every  colony  of 
Agaricia  agaricites  transplanted  next  to  Discosoma  sanc- 
tithomae was  damaged,  whereas  none  of  the  associated 
D.  sanctithomae  were  damaged.  Most  of  the  damage  oc- 
curred within  the  first  month  of  a  14-month  experimental 
period.  D.  sanctithomae  was  able  to  cause  severe  necrosis 
of  tissue  on  the  coral  Meandrina  meandhtcs,  which  is 
considered  to  be  near  the  top  of  the  Caribbean  coral  com- 
petitive hierarchy  due  to  its  effective  use  of  mesenterial 
filaments  in  damaging  other  scleractinians  (Lang,  1973). 
Although  many  of  the  D.  sanctithomae  were  initially  in- 
flicted with  extensive  body  lesions  by  M.  meandrites, 
many  recovered  and  retaliated  successfully,  causing  dam- 
age that  persisted  at  least  twelve  months.  This  represents 
a  clear  and  dramatic  example  of  a  competitive  reversal 
and  places  D.  sanctithomae  near  the  top  of  a  zoantharian 
competitive  hierarchy. 

Most  of  the  agonistic  behaviors  of  soft-bodied  antho- 
zoans  (and  some  scleractinians)  involve  morphological 
modifications  that  provide  the  capability  to  inflict  damage. 
Anemones  in  the  family  Actiniidae  inflate  acrorhagi  (Abel. 
1954;  Francis,  1973a;  Sebens,  1984);  acontiate  anemones 
in  several  families  develop  "fighting  tentacles"  from  feed- 
ing tentacles  (Purcell,  1977;  Purcell  and  Kitting,  1982; 
Kaplan,  1983).  D.  sanctithomae  uses  marginal  tentacles 
frequently  filled  with  mesenterial  filaments  and  ectoderm 
engorged  with  specialized  nematocysts  (den  Hartog,  1977, 
1980).  The  marginal  tentacles  changed  from  thin,  filiform 
appendages  to  bulbous  acrospheres  in  the  presence  of  A. 
agaricites  and  M.  meandrites.  The  initial  increase  in  vol- 
ume seems  to  be  due  to  the  influx  of  mesenterial  filaments, 
which  is  later  compounded  by  the  ectoderm  thickening 
with  nematocysts  (as  seen  by  den  Hartog,  1977).  Unlike 
the  acrorhagi  (Bonnin,  1964;  Bigger,  1980),  which  become 
inflated  with  each  aggressive  interaction,  the  marginal 
tentacles  of  D.  sanctithomae  remain  bulbous  once  en- 
larged. In  this  study,  the  most  extensive  acrospheres  were 
found  closest  to  the  site  of  interaction  with  the  scleractin- 
ians. Acrospheres  never  developed  in  D.  sanctithomae 
surrounded  only  by  algae,  nor  were  they  found  in  D. 
sanctithomae  adjacent  to  sponges,  tunicates,  or  other  non- 
cnidarian  neighbors  in  the  field  surveys. 

Every  incidence  of  damage  to  the  experimental  corals 
was  associated  with  the  presence  of  acrospheres  except  in 
one  case.  The  association  between  acrospheres  and  dam- 
age is  further  supported  by  a  decrease  in  the  amount  of 
time  before  damage  appeared  on  the  corals  next  to  D. 
sanctithomae  with  acrospheres  compared  to  those  that 
developed  acrospheres  during  the  experiment.  As  the  in- 
teractions progressed,  algae  may  have  acted  as  a  buffer  to 
contact  with  the  acrospheres.  After  algae  began  to  settle 


on  the  bare  coral  skeleton,  the  acrospheres  did  not  inten- 
sify further. 

Discosoma  sanctithomae  responded  to  the  corals  ad- 
jacent to  them  by  developing  acrospheres  and  by  inflicting 
damage.  However,  the  response  was  extremely  graded.  D. 
sanctithomae  reacted  to  Agaricia  agaricites  much  differ- 
ently than  it  did  to  Meandrina  meandrites.  In  general, 
the  interactions  with  A.  agaricites  appeared  to  be  much 
more  gradual,  progressing  slowly,  but  ultimately  resulting 
in  the  development  of  acrospheres  on  D.  sanctithomae 
and  damage  to  the  coral.  Conversely,  the  behavior  of  D. 
sanctithomae  next  to  M.  meandrites  was  much  more  dra- 
matic, responding  to  the  aggressive  actions  of  M.  mean- 
drites. Within  less  than  twenty-four  hours,  D.  sanctitho- 
mae individuals  had  been  damaged  extensively  and  were 
withdrawn,  some  for  several  days.  Those  that  later  recov- 
ered and  attacked  M.  meandrites  did  so  with  well-devel- 
oped acrospheres.  Often  the  marginal  tentacles  and  even 
the  rim  of  the  oral  disk  were  thickened  and  swollen  (pers. 
obs.),  presumably  filled  with  potent  nematocysts  (den 
Hartog,  1977)  (Fig.  2B). 

The  laboratory  experiments  and  the  Series  II  transplants 
served  to  elucidate  the  differences  in  Discosoma  sancti- 
thomae's  behaviors.  D.  sanctithomae'?:  response  to  adja- 
cent Meandrina  meandrites  was  immediate  and  severe. 
M.  meandrites'  quickness  in  extruding  mesenterial  fila- 
ments and  inflicting  damage  deterred  D.  sanctithomae 
from  approaching  M.  meandrites.  In  contrast,  D.  sane- 
til  homae  placed  next  to  Agaricia  agaricites  repeatedly  re- 
laxed and  expanded  its  disk  directly  on  top  of  A.  agari- 
cites'living  tissue.  D.  sanctithomae  did  not  appear  to  be 
adversely  affected  by  A.  agaricites,  although  it  did  peri- 
odically retract  away  from  contact  with  the  coral.  D.  sanc- 
tithomae could  cause  small  amounts  of  necrosis  to  A. 
agaricites'  tissue  which,  at  least  in  the  early  stages,  was 
often  recovered  by  the  coral.  However,  sometime  after 
prolonged  exposure  and  repeated  attacks,  the  A.  agaricites 
was  no  longer  able  to  regain  lost  tissue.  Not  long  after  this 
stage,  algae  (usually  a  green  alga)  began  to  settle  on  the 
bare  coral  skeleton.  These  dead  areas  persisted  for  the 
remainder  of  the  study  period. 

The  use  of  mesenterial  filaments  in  competitive  inter- 
actions is  well  documented  for  scleractinians.  Some  of  the 
most  aggressive  corals  use  exclusively  mesenterial  fila- 
ments for  defense  (Lang.  1971,  1973;  Logan,  1984).  The 
corallimorph  Corynactis  californica  extruded  mesenterial 
filaments  primarily  out  the  mouth  but  also  through  the 
body  wall  and  once  through  the  tentacles  during  agonistic 
encounters  (Chadwick,  1987).  Discosoma  sanctithomae 
invoked  two  mechanisms  for  inflicting  damage  that  reflect 
its  phylogenetic  relationship  with  the  scleractinians  and 
its  particular  tentacle  morphology.  Like  many  hard  corals, 
the  corallimorph  readily  emitted  mesenterial  filaments  out 
of  the  mouth  and  through  the  body  wall  when  disturbed. 


414 


J.  S.  MILES 


In  addition.  D.  sanclithomae  continually  transferred 
mesenterial  filaments  into  the  discal  and  marginal  ten- 
tacles, sometimes  passing  them  out  through  the  tips.  It  is 
not  yet  known  whether  there  are  permanent  holes  at  the 
tips  of  these  tentacles  through  which  the  mesenteries  can 
pass.  A  similar  movement  of  mesenterial  filaments  occurs 
in  the  discal  tentacles  of  Rhodactis  howesii  (Corallimor- 
pharia)  during  feeding  behaviors  (Hamner  and  Dunn, 
1980). 

This  regular  fluctuation  of  mesenterial  filaments  dispose 
Discosoma  sanctithomae  to  be  capable  of  quickly  sending 
mesenterial  filaments  into  and  out  of  the  tips  of  the  mar- 
ginal tentacles  when  it  is  involved  in  agonistic  encounters 
with  neighboring  species.  D.  sanclithomae  was  observed 
to  damage  adjacent  corals  with  mesenterial  filaments  from 
its  marginal  tentacles.  Although  most  incidences  of  dam- 
age occurred  after  acrospheres  had  formed,  some  corals 
showed  damage  before  this  time  that  could  have  been 
caused  by  mesenterial  filaments.  By  extruding  the  fila- 
ments out  the  tips  of  the  marginal  tentacles,  D.  sanctitho- 
mae increases  the  probability  of  the  filaments  landing  on 
the  tissue  of  the  opposing  organism.  It  is  a  behavior  that 
the  corallimorph  can  invoke  quickly  at  the  time  of  inter- 
action because  mesenterial  filaments  regularly  fluctuate 
in  and  out  of  the  marginal  tentacles.  Hence,  there  is  less 
time  between  the  recognition  of  a  competitor  and  the 
commencement  of  an  aggressive  response  than  would  be 
required  to  form  sweeper  or  fighting  tentacles. 

Den  Hartog  ( 1977)  determined  that  the  size  and  density 
of  holotrich  nematocysts  was  greater  in  bulbous  marginal 
tentacles  than  in  the  filiform  type.  This  study  confirms 
that  interactions  with  scleractinians  can  induce  the  for- 
mation of  bulbous  tentacles.  After  a  period  of  less  than  a 
week,  thin  transparent  marginal  tentacles  of  Discosoma 
sanctithomae  became  thickened  and  more  opaque  in  the 
presence  of  the  coral  colonies.  Some  tentacles  doubled  in 
thickness  and  became  opaque,  while  others  more  than 
tripled  their  girth  and  were  associated  with  a  greatly  thick- 
ened oral  rim.  A  few  tentacles  elongated,  forming  distinct 
tips  with  acrospheres  (Fig.  2).  All  forms  of  these  thickened 
marginal  tentacles  were  able  to  cause  necrosis  of  the  coral 
tissue. 

The  combination  of  mesenterial  filaments  and  acro- 
spheres enabled  D.  sanctithomae  to  respond  immediately, 
warding  orTimminent  damage,  and  to  develop  an  alternate 
form  of  defense  that  required  more  time  to  initiate.  D. 
sanctithomae  may  use  mesenterial  filaments  in  response 
to  adverse  interactions  of  short  temporal  scale,  and  re- 
serves acrosphere  formation,  involving  the  costly  con- 
struction of  new  tissue  and  many  nematocysts,  for  pro- 
longed interactions.  Its  soft  body  allows  it  to  avoid  some 
acts  of  aggression  from  opponents  by  bending  away  and 
perhaps  even  by  moving  the  base  laterally.  All  such  char- 
acteristics make  D.  sanctithomae  an  effective  competitor. 


holding  its  own  on  the  substratum  of  the  reef  against  some 
of  the  most  aggressive  corals. 

Acknowledgments 

This  work  was  supported  partially  by  the  Lerner-Gray 
Fund  for  Marine  Research  and  by  Sigma  Xi  Grants-in- 
Aid  of  Research.  My  research  was  aided  by  the  field  as- 
sistance of  Lars  Kula,  Pete  Edmunds,  Ruth  Gates,  and 
many  members  of  Northeastern  University's  East/West 
Program.  P.  Schultze  provided  insightful  discussion  on 
the  statistical  analysis.  K.  P.  Sebens,  J.  Witman,  M.  P. 
Morse,  and  D.  O'Brien,  along  with  two  anonymous  re- 
viewers critically  reviewed  the  manuscript.  Thank  you 
all.  This  is  contribution  number  182,  Marine  Science 
Center,  Northeastern  University,  Nahant,  MA,  and  num- 
ber 495,  Discovery  Bay  Marine  Laboratory,  Discovery 
Bay,  Jamaica. 

Literature  Cited 

Abel,  E.  F.  1954.     Ein  Beilrag  zur  Giftwirkung  der  Aktmien  und  Funk- 

tion  der  Randsackchen.  Zool.  An:.  153:  19-268. 
Abel,  E.  F.  1970.     Uber  den  Tentakelapparat  der  Edelkoralle  (Corallhim 

nihriim  L.)  und  seine  Funktion  beim  Beutefangverhalten.  Oecologia 

4:  133-142. 
Bak,  R.  P.  M.,  and  J.  L.  A.  Borsboom.  1984.     Allelopathic  interaction 

between  a  reef  coelenterate  and  benthic  algae.  Oecologia  63:  194- 

198. 
Bak,  R.  P.  M.,  R.  M.  Termaat,  and  R.  Dekker.  1982.     Complexity  of 

coral  interactions:  influence  of  time,  location  of  interaction  and  epi- 

fauna.  Mar.  Biol  69:  215-222. 
Bigger,  C.  H.  1980.     Interspecific  and  intraspecific  acrorhagial  aggressive 

behavior  among  sea  anemones:  a  recognition  of  self  and  not-self. 

Biol.  Bull.  159:  117-134. 
Hi  HIM  1 1 1.  J.  P.  1964.     Recherches  sur  la  "reaction  d'aggression"  et  sur  le 

fonctionnement  des  aerorrhages  d' 'Actinia  equina  L.  Bull.  Biol.  Fr. 

Belg.  98:  225-250. 
Brace,  R.  C.  1981.     Intraspecific  aggression  in  the  color  morphs  of  the 

anemone  Phymactis  clematis  from  Chile.  Mar.  Biol.  64:  85-93. 
Brace,  R.  C.,  and  J.  Pavey.  1978.     Size  dependent  dominance  hierarchy 

in  the  anemone  Actinia  equina.  Nature  273:  752-753. 
Chadwick,  N.  1987.     Interspecific  aggressive  behavior  of  the  coralli- 

morpharian  Corynactis  califonuca  (Cnidaria:  Anthozoa):  Effects  on 

sympatnc  corals  and  sea  anemones.  Biol.  Bull.  173:  1 10-125. 
Chornesky,  E.  A.  1985.     Repeated  reversals  of  competitive  dominance 

during  spatial  competition  between  reef  corals.  Ecology  70(4):  843- 

855. 
Chornesky,  E.  A.  1983.     Induced  development  ol  sweeper  tentacles  on 

the  reef  coral  Agaricia  agariciles:  a  response  to  direct  competition. 

Biol.  Bull.  165:569-581. 
Chornesky,  E.  A.,  and  S.  L.  Williams.  1983.     Distribution  of  sweeper 

tentacles  on  Monlaslrea  cavernosa.  Pp.  61-67  in  The  Ecology  of 

Deep  and  Shallow  Reefs,  M.  L.  Reaka,  ed.  Symp.  Ser.  Undersea  Res.. 

N.O.A.A.  Nat.  Undersea  Res.  Prog..  1. 
den  Hartog,  J.  C.  1980.     Caribbean  shallow  water  Corallimorphana. 

Zool,  Verli   176:  1-83. 

den  Hartog,  J.  C.  1977.     The  marginal  tentacles  of  Rhodactis  sanctitho- 
mae (Corallimorphana)  sweeper  tentacles  of  Monlaslrea  cavernosa 

(Scleractinia).  their  cnidom  and  possible  function.  The  Second  Int. 

Coral  Reef  Symposium  Univ.  Miami  Press,  Miami.  FL.  1:  463-470. 


CORALL1MORPHARIAN  AGGRESSION 


415 


Francis,  I,.  I973a.  Clone  specific  segregation  in  the  sea  anemone  An- 
iliopleura  elegantissima.  Bio/.  Bid  I  1-44:  64-72. 

Francis,  I,.  I973b.  Intraspedfic  aggression  and  its  effects  on  the  dis- 
tribution of  Anthopleura  elegantissima  and  some  related  sea  ane- 
mones. Biol.  Bull  1-44:  73-92. 

Goreau,  T.  F.  1959.  The  ecology  of  Jamaican  coral  reefs  I.  Species 
composition  and  zonation.  Ecology  40:  67-90. 

Goreau,  I .  F.,  and  N.  I.  Goreau.  1973.  The  ecology  of  Jamaican  coral 
reefs  II.  Geomorphology,  zonation.  and  sedimentary  phases.  Bull. 
Mm  Sa.  23:  399-464. 

Hamner,  VV.  M.,  and  D.  F.  Dunn.  1980.  Tropical  Corallimorphana 
(Coelenterata:  Anthozoal:  feeding  by  envelopment.  Micronesica,  vol 
16,  June. 

Ilidaka,  M.,  and  K.  Yamazato.  1984.  Intraspecific  interactions  in  a 
scleractinian  coral.  Galaxeafascicidaris  induced  formation  of  sweeper 
tentacles.  Coral  Reels  3:  77-85. 

I  lildi  in.iiiii.  W.  H.  1974.  Some  new  concepts  in  immunological  phy- 
logeny.  Nature  250:  116-120. 

Kaplan,  8.  A.  1983.  Intrasexual  aggression  in  Mclridnim  senile.  Biol. 
Bull.  165:  416-418. 

LaBarrc,  S.  1986.  Competitive  strategies  of  soft  corals  (Coelenterata: 
Octocorallia):  III.  Spacing  and  aggressive  interactions  between  al- 
cyonaceans.  Mar  Ecol.  Prog  Set:  28:  147-156. 

Lang,  J.  C.  1971.  Interspecific  aggression  by  scleractinian  corals  I.  The 
rediscovery  of  Scolvmia  cubensis  (Milne  Edwards  &  Haime).  Bull 
Mar  Sa  21:  952-959. 

Lang,  J.  C.  1973.  Interspecific  aggression  by  scleractinian  reef  corals 
II.  Why  the  race  is  not  only  to  the  swift.  Bull.  Mar.  Sci  23:  260- 
279. 

Lang,  J.  C.,  and  E.  A.  Chorneskj.  1988.  Competition  between  scler- 
actinian reef  corals:  a  review  of  mechanisms  and  effects.  In  Ecosystems 
of  I/if  World.  Coral  Reefs.  Z.  Dubinsky.  ed.  Elsevier  Press,  Amster- 
dam. 

Liddell,  W.  D..  and  S.  L.  Ohlhorst.  1986.  Changes  in  the  benthic  com- 
munity composition  following  the  mass  mortality  of  Diadenia  at 
Jamaica.  J.  E.\p  Mar  Biol.  Ecol.  95:  271-278. 

Liddell,  VV.  D.,  S.  L.  Ohlhorst,  and  A.  G.  Choates.  1984.  Modern  and 
Ancient  Carbonate  Environments  of  Jamaica  Sedementa  X.  Miami 
Beach.  Univ.  of  Florida  Press.  99  pp. 

Logan,  A.  1984.  Interspecific  aggression  in  herrnatypic  corals  from 
Bermuda.  Coral  Reefs  3:  131-138. 


Mn/ik.  K.  1983.     Zoom  and  focus:  octocorals.  Newton  Graphic  Science 

Magazine  I  Japan)  3:  30-35. 
Pawlik,  S.  R.,  M.  I .  Burch,  and  \V.  Kenical.  1987.     Patterns  of  chemical 

defense  among  Caribbean  gorgonian  corals:  a  preliminary  survey.  J. 

E.\p.  Mar.  Biol  Ecol.  108:  55-66. 
Purcell,  J.  E.  1977.     Aggressive  function  and  induced  development  of 

catch  tentacles  in  the  sea  anemone  Metrulnini  senile  Coelenterata: 

Actimaria.  Biol.  Bull  153:  355-368. 
Purcell,  J.  E.,  and  C.  L.  Killing.  1982.     Intraspecific  aggression  and 

population  distributions  of  the  sea  anemone  Metridinm  senile.  Biol 

Bull  162:  345-359. 
Richardson,  C.  A.,  P.  Dustan,  and  J.  C.  Lang.  1979.     Maintenance  of 

living  space  by  sweeper  tentacles  ofMontastrea  cavernosa,  a  Caribbean 

reef  coral.  Mar.  Biol  55:  181-186. 
Rogers,  C.  S.,  M.  Gilnack,  and  H.  C.  Fitz  III.  1983.     Monitoring  of 

coral  reefs  with  linear  transects:  a  study  of  storm  damage.  J.  E.\p. 

.Mar.  Biol.  Ecol  66:  285-300. 

Sammarco,  P.  W.,  J.  C.  Coll,  and  S.  LaBarre.  1985.     Competitive  strat- 
egies of  soft  corals  (Coelenterata:  Octocorallia).  Coral  Reefs  1:  173- 

178. 
Sammarco,     P.    W.,    J.    C.    Coll,    S.     LaBarre,    and     B.    Willis. 

1983.     Competitive  strategies  of  soft  corals  (Coelenterata:  Octocor- 
allia): allelopathic  effects  on  selected  scleractinian  corals.  J.  E.\p.  Mar. 

Biol  Ecol  91:  199-215. 
Sauer,  K.  P..  M.  Muller,  and  M.  Weber.  1986.     Alloimmune  memory 

for  glycoproteid  recognition  molecules  in  sea  anemones  competing 

for  space.  Mar  Biol  92:  73-79. 
Sebens,  K.  P.  1976.     The  ecology  of  Caribbean  sea  anemones  in  Panama: 

utilization  of  space  on  a  coral  reef.  Pp.  67-77  in  Coelenterate  Ecology 

and  Behavior.  G.  O.  Mackie.  ed.  Plenum  Publ.,  NY. 
Sebens,  K.  P.  1984.     Agonistic  behavior  in  the  intertidal  sea  anemone 

Anthopleura  xanthogrammica.  Biol  Bull.  166:  457-472. 
Sebens,  K.  P.,  and  J.  S.  Miles.  1988.     Sweeper  tentacles  in  a  gorgonian 

octocoral:  morphological  modifications  for  interference  competition. 

Biol.  Bull.  175:  378-387. 
Theodor,  J.  L.  1970.     Distinction  between  "self"  and  "not  self  in  lower 

invertebrates.  Nature  221:  690-692. 
Williams,  R.  B.  1975.     Catch  tentacles  in  sea  anemones:  occurrence  in 

Haliplanella  luciae  (Verrill)  and  a  review  of  current  knowledge.  / 

Nat.  Hist.  9:  241-248. 


Reference:  Biol.  Bull.  180:  416-431.  (June,  1991) 


Autotomy  in  Blue  Crab  (Callinectes  sapidus  Rathbun) 

Populations:  Geographic,  Temporal, 

and  Ontogenetic  Variation 

L.  DAVID  SMITH12  AND  ANSON  H.  MINES' 

^Smithsonian  Environmental  Research  Center,  P.O.  Box  28,  Edgewater.  Maryland  21037  and 
"-Department  of  Zoology,  University  of  Maryland,  College  Park,  Maryland,  20742 


Abstract.     Blue  crab  (Callinectes  sapidus  Rathbun)  pop- 
ulations were  examined  at  four  sites  in  Chesapeake  Bay 
and  three  additional  sites  along  the  southeastern  Atlantic 
coast  and  Gulf  of  Mexico;  the  aims  were  to  assess  the 
incidence  of  limb  autotomy  and  to  determine  whether 
injury  patterns  varied  temporally,  geographically,  and 
ontogenetically.  These  data,  which  include  four  years  of 
information  from  one  site  (Rhode  River,  Maryland,  a  su- 
bestuary  of  central  Chesapeake  Bay),  make  this  study  the 
most  extensive  and  intensive  survey  of  limb  autotomy  yet 
conducted  in  arthropods.  A  substantial  percentage  ( 1 7- 
39%)  of  the  blue  crab  populations  were  either  missing  or 
regenerating  one  or  more  limbs,  suggesting  that  autotomy 
is  an  important  mechanism  for  their  survival.  The  fre- 
quency of  limb  autotomy  varied,  both  within  and  between 
years,  and  over  broad  geographical  scales.  Injury  levels 
were  generally  correlated  positively  with  crab  size.  Limb 
autotomy  was  independent  of  sex  and  molt  stage,  and 
frequencies  varied  little  among  sites  in  the  Rhode  River. 
Patterns  of  limb  injury  in  C.  sapidus  were  remarkably 
consistent  among  all  sites.  The  most  frequent  injury  in- 
volved loss  of  a  single  cheliped.  Swimming  legs  suffered 
the  least  damage.  Severe  multiple  limb  loss  was  rare.  Right 
and  left  limbs  were  lost  with  equal  frequency  in  most 
populations.  This  consistency  of  autotomy  pattern  sug- 
gests differential  vulnerability  of  limbs  and  standard  be- 
havioral response  by  blue  crabs  to  various  injury-causing 
agents.  The  frequency  of  autotomy  was  density-dependent 
in  the  Rhode  River,  indicating  that  intraspecinc  interac- 
tions (e.g.,  cannibalism)  may  be  a  major  cause  of  limb 


Received  2  October  1990:  accepted  27  December  1990. 


loss  in  populations  in  the  Rhode  River  subestuary  and 
elsewhere. 

Introduction 

Many  invertebrate  and  vertebrate  species  respond  to 
injury  or  its  threat  by  autotomizing  (i.e..  severing)  a  body 
part  along  a  breakage  plane  (Wood  and  Wood,  1932; 
Needham,  1953;  Robinson  et  a!..  1970;  Vitt  et  al.  1977; 
McVean,  1982;  McCallum  et  al..  1989).  While  such  be- 
havior has  immediate  survival  benefits  (Dial  and  Fitzpa- 
trick,  1983;  Medel  et  al..  1988;  Smith,  1990a),  autotomy 
may  handicap  individuals  when  foraging  (Slater  and 
Lawrence,  1980;  Smith,  1990a),  overwintering  (Willis  et 
al..  1982);  escaping  predators  (Vitt  et  al..  1977;  Dial  and 
Fitzpatrick,  1984;  Smith,  1990a),  or  competing  for  mates 
(Sekkelsten,  1988;  Smith,  1 990a)  or  shelter  (Conover  and 
Miller,  1978;BerzinsandCaldwell,  1983).  Energetic  costs 
of  regenerating  body  parts  can  reduce  reproductive  output 
(Maiorana,  1977)  and  growth  (Kuris  and  Mager,  1975; 
Smith,  1990b).  Theoretical  models  (Harris,  1989)  have 
suggested  that  nonlethal  injury  could  regulate  population 
abundance,  if  injury  rates  were  density-dependent  and 
significantly  reduced  long-term  survival  or  reproduction. 
Detailed  knowledge  of  autotomy  patterns  and  frequencies, 
for  a  single  species,  over  both  narrow  and  broad  temporal 
and  geographic  scales,  are  needed  to  make  more  reliable 
inferences  concerning  the  fitness  benefits  and  conse- 
quences of  autotomy. 

Quantitative  surveys  of  limb  loss  in  decapod  crusta- 
ceans exist  for  only  a  few  species  (Cancer  magister,  Durkin 
et  al..  1984;  Shirley  and  Shirley,  1988;  Cancer  pagurus. 
Bennett.  1973;  Carcinus  maenas,  Needham,  1953; 
McVean,  1976;  McVean  and  Findlay,  1979;  Sekkelsten, 


416 


AUTOTOMY   IN  BLUE  CRAB  POPULATIONS 


417 


1988,  Menippe  mercenaria,  Sullivan,  1979;Simonson  and 
Steele,  1981:  Simonson,  1985;  Paralithodes  camtschatica 
and  Chionoceles  bairdi,  Edwards,  1972).  The  percentage 
of  injury  in  these  species  ranged  from  13-66%.  Inferences 
from  these  data  regarding  the  fitness  consequences  of  au- 
totomy  have  been  limited,  however,  because  field  data 
have  not  been  collected  for  more  than  one  complete 
growing  season;  smaller  individuals  in  commercial  species 
frequently  have  not  been  sampled;  chelipeds  have  often 
been  the  only  limbs  assessed;  and  collections  have  been 
geographically  restricted.  To  understand  how  the  inci- 
dence of  autotomy  varies  within  and  among  populations, 
multiple-year  and  -site  data  on  injury  are  needed  for  a 
range  of  body  sizes  for  both  sexes. 

Nonlethal  injury  often  results  from  unsuccessful  attacks 
by  predators  (Vermeij.  1982).  Variation  in  injury  levels 
among  populations  and  species  has  been  thought  to  reflect 
differences  in  predation  intensity  and  efficiency  over  al- 
titudinal  gradients  (Ballinger,  1979;  Shaffer,  1978).  eco- 
logical habitats  (Schoener  and  Schoener.  1980);  biogeo- 
graphic  regions  (Vermeij,  1976);  geologic  time  (Vermeij, 
1977,  1983),  life  histories  (Vittrffl/..  1977),  and  behaviors 
(Jaksic  and  Fuentes,  1980;  Schall  and  Pianka,  1980).  Al- 
though specific  agents  responsible  for  autotomy  in  nature 
are  rarely  identified  (cf.,  Robinson  ct  a/..  1970;  Jaeger, 
1981:  Smith,  1990a),  such  information  is  needed  to  un- 
derstand the  patterns  and  impact  of  injury  in  populations. 
Intraspecific  predation  is  common  in  the  animal  kingdom 
(Fox,  1975;Polis,  1 98 1;  Stevens  etai.  1982;Reaka,  1987; 
Kurihara  and  Okamoto,  1987),  and  may  be  an  important 
cause  of  autotomy  in  some  taxa  (e.g.,  salamanders;  Jaeger, 
1981).  Large  Callinectes  sapidns  are  known  to  prey  on 
smaller  conspecifics  (Laughlin,  1982;  Hines  ct  at..  1990; 
Peery,  1989;  Smith,  1990a).  If  intraspecific  interactions 
are  chiefly  responsible  for  autotomy  in  blue  crabs,  then 
injury  levels  should  correlate  positively  with  population 
densities  over  temporal  and  spatial  scales. 

Costs  of  nonlethal  injury  to  individuals  will  depend  on 
the  type  and  number  of  missing  limbs.  The  relative  im- 
portance of  different  limbs  to  survival,  in  turn,  may  be 
indicated  by  the  frequency  of  their  repair  in  the  popula- 
tion. Limb  regeneration  in  arthropods  occurs  upon  molt- 
ing, and  crabs  may  require  a  number  of  molts  (e.g..  1-3 
in  Callinectes  sapidux.  Smith,  1990b;  >4  in  Paralithodes 
camtschatica.  Edwards,  1972)  before  full  limb  length  is 
restored.  For  most  limbs,  evidence  of  past  injury  disap- 
pears once  symmetry  has  been  restored.  Following  the 
loss  of  a  major  (crusher)  claw,  however,  normal  cheliped 
dimorphism  is  often  not  reestablished  (Smith,  1990b); 
thus,  the  absence  of  such  dimorphism  can  serve  as  a  mea- 
sure of  survival  of  past  injury. 

To  assess  the  impact  of  autotomy  in  a  population,  it  is 
necessary  to:  ( 1 )  document  spatial,  temporal,  and  onto- 


genetic  variation  in  patterns  and  levels  of  injury;  (2)  iden- 
tify causal  agents;  and  (3)  determine  the  various  costs  of 
injury  to  individuals.  The  present  study  examines  inci- 
dences of  autotomy  in  blue  crabs  (Callinectes  sapidns 
Rathbun)  at  four  sites  in  Chesapeake  Bay  and  three  ad- 
ditional sites  along  the  southeastern  LJnited  States  Atlantic 
coast  and  the  Gulf  of  Mexico.  These  data,  which  include 
four  years  of  information  from  one  site  in  central  Ches- 
apeake Bay,  make  this  the  most  detailed  survey  yet  con- 
ducted on  autotomy  in  arthropods. 

Materials  and  Methods 

Sampling  procedures 

Callinectes  sapidns  individuals  were  collected  from 
1986  to  1989  in  the  Rhode  River,  Maryland;  at  three 
additional  sites  in  the  Chesapeake  Bay  in  fall  1989;  and 
at  three  sites  along  the  southeastern  Atlantic  coast  and 
the  Gulf  of  Mexico  of  the  United  States  (Figs.  1,  2)  in 
spring  1989.  At  all  locations,  crabs  were  measured  or  ex- 
amined for:  ( 1 )  carapace  width  between  tips  of  lateral 
spines.  (2)  sex,  (3)  sexual  maturity  in  females  (1986-89) 
and  males  (1988-89,  only),  (4)  molt  stage,  (5)  type  and 
side  of  any  missing  or  regenerating  limbs,  (6)  lengths  of 
limb  buds,  regenerating  limbs,  and  contralateral  intact 
limbs,  and  (7)  side  of  the  crusher  claw. 

Sexual  maturity  in  female  blue  crabs  was  determined 
by  examining  differences  in  abdominal  allometry  (Van 
Engel,  1958).  For  males,  sexual  maturity  was  indicated 
by  the  ease  with  which  the  abdomen  could  be  pulled  away 
from  the  ventral  surface  of  the  cephalothorax  (Van  Engel, 
1958;  1990).  Molt  stages  were  determined  by  assessing 
carapace  hardness  and  by  examining  the  propodus  of  the 
fifth  pereopod  for  evidence  of  epidermal  retraction  (Van 
Engel,  1958;  Johnson,  1980).  A  limb  stump  that  was  either 
scarred,  or  possessed  a  papilla  or  limb  bud,  was  classified 
as  a  missing  limb.  A  regenerating  limb  was  considered  to 
be  a  functional  appendage  that  had  undergone  at  least 
one  molt  since  autotomy,  but  was  shorter  than  the  intact, 
contralateral  limb.  Crabs  that  possessed  an  unscarred 
stump  wound,  indicating  possible  injury  caused  during 
collection,  were  not  measured.  Limb  length  was  measured 
as  the  distance  from  the  autotomy  plane  in  the  basi-ischial 
segment  to  the  dactyl  tip  of  a  fully  extended  limb. 

Site  descriptions  and  collection  methods 

Rhode  River.  Alary/and.  Callinectes  sapidns  individuals 
were  collected  from  the  Rhode  River  near  Edgewater, 
Maryland  (38°51'N,  76°32'W),  between  July  and  No- 
vember in  1986,  and  from  May  to  November  each  year 
from  1987  to  1989  (Figs.  1,  2).  The  Rhode  River  is  a 


418 


L.  D.  SMITH  AND  A.  H.  MINES 


Chesapeake  Bay  Region 


Rhode  River,  Md 

and 
Upper-rrtd  bay,  Md 


Patuxent  River,  Md 


Lower-rnkJ  bay,  Va 


Atlantic  Ocean 


North  Wet,  SC 


Irxian  Rtver,  R 


Figure  1.     Map  of  the  United  States  Atlantic  coast  and  the  Gulf  of  Mexico  showing  locations  of  blue 
crab  sampling  sites  from  1986  to  1989. 


shallow  (maximum  depth  =  4  m),  485  ha  mesohaline 
subestuary  that  empties  into  the  western  side  of  the  upper- 
central  Chesapeake  Bay  (Hines  el  al,  1987a,b).  Water 
temperatures  ranged  from  8°C  to  34°C  during  the  sam- 
pling period,  with  July  temperatures  averaging  28°C 
(±2.4).  Salinities  typically  ranged  from  4  to  14%o;  but 
unusually  low  salinities  (0-10%o)  were  recorded  in 
1989. 

Several  methods  were  used  to  sample  blue  crabs  in  the 
Rhode  River  as  well  as  at  other  sites.  Potential  biases  re- 
lated to  these  different  collection  techniques  were  exam- 
ined and  are  discussed  below.  In  all  four  years,  blue  crabs 
were  sampled  monthly  by  otter  trawl  (3  m  wide  mouth; 
5  mm  mesh  net  body;  7  mm  mesh  cod  end;  with  tickler 
chain;  Hines  el  al.,  1987a)  pulled  for  900  m  on  two  con- 


secutive days  at  each  of  three  stations  in  the  Rhode  River. 
Two  stations  were  located  at  the  river  mouth,  one  over 
sandy  substrate,  and  the  other  over  muddy  sediment;  a 
third  station  was  located  at  the  river  head  over  muddy 
sediment  (Fig.  2). 

From  1986  to  1988,  blue  crabs  were  also  collected  bi- 
weekly at  a  fish  weir  spanning  the  principal  freshwater 
tributary  (Muddy  Creek)  of  the  Rhode  River.  Crabs  mov- 
ing up-  and  downstream  were  captured  separately  in  single 
hoop  nets  (7  mm  mesh).  No  crabs  were  sampled  at  the 
weir  in  1989  because  of  storm-related  damage.  Conse- 
quently, in  1989,  crabs  were  collected  biweekly  at  the  river 
head  (0.5-2  m  depth);  larger  individuals  were  caught  in 
baited  commercial  crab  pots  (57  mm  mesh),  and  smaller 
crabs  in  specially  designed  crab  pots  (7  mm  mesh).  Blue 


AUTOTOMY  IN  BLUE  CRAB  POPULATIONS 


419 


. —  '     Rjver 

Weir  •    .         Head 

Trawl 


Figure  2.  Map  of  the  Rhode  River  subestuary,  Maryland,  showing 
sampling  sites  from  1986  to  1989.  These  include  mouth  sand  and  mouth 
mud  trawl  stations  ( 1986-89)  =  River  Mouth;  nver  head  trawl  station 
( 1986-89)  and  crab  pot/seine  sites  (1989,  only)  =  River  Head;  and  Muddy 
Creek  up-  and  downstream  weir  nets  (1986-88)  =  Creek. 


crabs  were  also  sampled  periodically  in  nearshore  waters 
(depth  0.3-1.2  m)  with  a  10  m  beach  seine  (1  mm  mesh). 
Non-Rhode  River  sites.  Locations,  dates,  physical  con- 
ditions (e.g..  depth,  salinity),  and  sampling  techniques  for 


additional  sites  in  Chesapeake  Bay  and  for  sites  in  South 
Carolina,  Florida,  and  Alabama  are  summarized  in  Table 
I.  Note  that  the  upper-mid  Chesapeake  Bay  site  was  only 
1  km  east  of  the  mouth  of  the  Rhode  River,  Maryland. 
Using  a  dipnet,  blue  crabs  were  collected  from  the  sides 
of  a  commercial  pound  net  at  this  site. 

Statistical  analyses 

Data  were  treated  as  categorical,  and  frequencies  were 
analyzed  by  logistic  regressions  (Cox,  1970;  PROC  CAT- 
MOD  with  maximum  likelihood  estimation,  0.5  added 
to  all  cells;  SAS  Institute,  1985)  or  two-way  contingency 
tables.  In  the  Rhode  River,  data  were  analyzed  for  only 
those  months  when  25  or  more  crabs  were  obtained.  Lo- 
cations in  the  Rhode  River  were  combined  into  river 
mouth,  river  head,  or  creek  sites,  because  differences  in 
autotomy  frequency  were  not  detected  between  sample 
stations  within  each  subregion  (G-tests,  P  >  0.05).  Crabs 
were  divided  into  small  (<61  mm  carapace  width),  me- 
dium (61  <  CW  <  1 10  mm),  and  large  (>1 10  mm  CW) 
size  classes.  The  division  between  medium  and  large  size 
classes  corresponded  approximately  with  the  onset  of  sex- 
ual maturity.  Molt  stages  were  classified  as  postmolt  (stages 
A  and  B),  intermolt  (stage  C),  and  premolt  (stage  D) 
(Johnson,  1980).  Crabs  in  the  act  of  molting  (stage  E) 
were  very  rare  and  were  included  as  premolt  animals. 

The  primary  null  hypothesis  tested  whether  the  fre- 
quency of  injured  crabs  (i.e.,  those  animals  missing  or 
regenerating  at  least  one  limb)  in  a  population  was  in- 
dependent of  one  or  more  of  the  following  independent 
variables:  ( 1 )  year  (Rhode  River,  only),  (2)  month  (Rhode 


Table  1 

Sampling  sites,  dales,  physical  conditions,  and  sampling  methods  used  to  collect  blue  crabs  in  1989 


Location 

Sampling 

Depth 

Salinity 

Temp. 

Sampling 

Site 

(Lat.,  Long.) 

dates  1989 

(m) 

(%») 

(°C) 

method 

Upper-  Mid 

38°50'N,  76°31'W 

Aug.-Sept. 

5 

6-11 

24-28 

dip  net 

Chesapeake  Bay, 

Maryland 

Patuxent  River, 

38°23'N,  76°36'W 

Oct. 

9-21 

12-14 

14 

otter  trawl 

Maryland 

Lower-Mid  Chesapeake 
Bay,  Virginia 

North  Inlet. 
South  Carolina 

Indian  River,  Florida 
Mobile  Bay,  Alabama 


37°25'N-37039'N 
75°56'W-76017'W 

33°2nM,  79°11'W 


27°50'N,  80°29'W 
SO'MS'N,  88°00'W 


Oct. 
May 

May 

May 


4-18 

.2-3 

.5-5 
.5-5 


19 
21 

23-26 
24 


19 
31 

29-32 

27 


otter  trawl 

crab  pots,  dip  net 

crab  pots,  dip  net 
crab  pots,  seine 


Latitudinal  and  longitudinal  range  of  sampling  transects  are  given  for  the  lower-mid  Chesapeake  Bay  site.  See  text  for  description  of  1986-1989 
Rhode  River  surveys. 


420 


L.  D.  SMITH  AND  A.  H.  MINES 

Table  II 
's  and  percentages  of  crabs  missing,  regenerating,  ami  both  missing  and  regenerating  lunhx  in  the  Rhode  River  from  1986  lo  19S9 


Rhode  River,  Maryland 


1986" 


1987" 


1988" 


Category' 


1  Missing  =  crabs  with  one  or  more  scarred  stumps,  papillae,  or  limb  buds. 

2  Regenerating  =  crabs  possessing  one  or  more  functional  but  shortened  appendages. 

3  Miss.  +  Regen.  =  crabs  possessing  both  missing  and  regenerating  limbs. 

4  Size  ratio:  (S  <  61  mm  carapace  width,  M  =  61-1 10  mm  CW.  L  >  1 10  mm  CW)  for  all  crabs  (injured  and  intact). 
Years  with  the  same  superscripted  letter  did  not  differ  significantly  in  total  autotomy  frequency. 


1989" 


Total  intact 

1050 

75.0 

505 

81.2 

536 

82.5 

569 

82.2 

Total  injured: 

350 

25.0 

117 

18.8 

113 

17.5 

123 

17.8 

Missing' 

211 

15.1 

56 

9.0 

53 

8.2 

68 

9.8 

Regenerating2 

123 

8.8 

55 

8.8 

57 

8.8 

46 

6.7 

Miss.  +  Regen.3 

16 

1.1 

6 

1.0 

3 

0.5 

9 

1.3 

Total  caught 

1400 

100.0 

622 

100.0 

649 

100.0 

692 

100.0 

Sex  ratio  M:F 

67:33 

81:19 

83:17 

76:24 

Size  ratio  S:M:L4 

12:48:40 

20:26:54 

21:38:41 

32:18:50 

River,  only),  (3)  subestuarine  location  (Rhode  River, 
only).  (4)  body  size,  (5)  sex,  (6)  sexual  maturity,  (7)  molt 
stage,  and  (8)  geographic  location.  All  relevant  two  factor 
combinations  of  these  independent  variables  were  tested 
by  logistic  regression  for  their  relationship  to  the  binary 
response  variable  (i.e..  frequency  of  injured  versus  unin- 
jured crabs).  Expected  cell  frequencies  of  injured  animals 
were  often  low  (<1)  and  prevented  more  than  two  inde- 
pendent variables  from  being  tested  reliably  in  a  single 
model.  Significant  two-way  interactions  were  not  recorded 
between  independent  variables  in  most  instances;  hence, 
these  results,  except  when  specified,  are  not  discussed.  If 
a  test  revealed  nonindependence.  unplanned  multiple 
comparisons  controlling  for  experimentwise  type  I  error 
were  used  to  distinguish  differences  among  frequencies 
(simultaneous  test  procedures,  STP  tests;  Sokal  and  Rohlf, 
198 1,  pp.  728).  Two-way  contingency  tables  were  used  to 
examine  frequencies  of  injury  as  a  function  of  limb  type 
and  number,  right  versus  left  side,  and  missing  versus  re- 
generating limbs. 

Median  carapace  widths  of  injured  and  uninjured  an- 
imals were  compared  within  sites  by  nonparametric  pro- 
cedures (Mann-Whitney  U-test;  Sokal  and  Rohlf,  1981), 
because  variances  for  carapace  widths  were  heteroscedastic 
(F-max  test;  Sokal  and  Rohlf,  1981)  even  after  attempts 
at  data  transformation. 

Results 

Population  structure 

In  the  Rhode  River,  sex  ratios  were  consistently  male- 
dominated,  but  relative  frequencies  of  males  and  females 


differed  among  all  years  except  between  1987  and  1988 
(STP  test,  3  df;  Table  II).  Annual  size-frequency  distri- 
butions differed  among  all  years  in  the  Rhode  River  and 
among  all  other  sites  in  1 989  (Komolgorov-Smirnov  two- 
sample  tests,  P  <  0.05;  Tables  II,  III).  Outside  the  Rhode 
River,  sex  ratios  were  skewed  towards  females  at  all  sites 
except  South  Carolina  and  the  Patuxent  River,  Maryland 
(G-test,  5  df,  P  <  0.05;  Table  III).  Collections  from  the 
upper-mid  Chesapeake  Bay  were  designed  to  capture  fe- 
males and  larger  individuals;  therefore,  these  sex  and  size 
ratios  should  not  be  compared  to  those  from  other  sites, 
which  were  sampled  randomly. 

Sampling  methods 

The  frequency  of  autotomy  in  crabs  collected  from 
baited  crab  pots  and  seines  at  the  Rhode  River  head  (19%) 
did  not  differ  from  injury  levels  in  otter  trawls  (22%)  at 
that  site  in  1989  (G-test,  1  df,  P  >  0.05).  No  significant 
differences  in  injury  were  observed  between  otter  trawl 
and  fish  weir  collections  from  1986  to  1988  (G-tests,  1  df, 
P  >  0.05).  At  non-Rhode  River  sites,  autotomy  frequen- 
cies did  not  differ  among  crabs  collected  by  otter  trawl 
(Patuxent  River,  lower-mid  Chesapeake  Bay)  and  crab 
pots  and  seines  (South  Carolina,  Florida,  Alabama)  (G- 
tests,  P>  0.05). 

A  utotomy  frequencies 

Yearly  and  geographic  variation.  Frequencies  of  blue 
crabs  missing  or  regenerating  one  or  more  limbs  differed 
significantly  among  sites  and  years  sampled  (G-tests,  P 


AUTOTOMY  IN  BLUE  CRAB  POPULATIONS 


421 


Table  III 

Frequencies  and  percentages  of  crabs  missing,  regenerating,  and  both  missing  and  regenerating  timbs  at  sites 
in  the  Chesapeake  Bay  and  along  the  southeastern  United  Stales  in  1 989 

Chesapeake  Bay 


Upper-Mida 

Patuxent  R." 

Lower-Mid  h 

N. 

Inlet  SCb 

Indian  R.  FL" 

Mobile  B. 
AL'b 

Category 

n 

% 

n 

% 

n 

% 

n 

% 

n 

% 

n 

% 

Total  intact 

549 

80.9 

63 

61.2 

150 

67.0 

139 

68.1 

132 

65.7 

191 

73.5 

Total  injured: 

130 

19.1 

40 

38.8 

74 

33.0 

65 

31.9 

69 

34.3 

69 

26.5 

Missing' 

85 

12.5 

24 

23.3 

53 

23.7 

33 

16.2 

37 

IX.4 

25 

9.6 

Regenerating2 

39 

5.7 

1  1 

10.6 

17 

7.6 

27 

13.2 

32 

15.9 

40 

15.4 

Miss.  +  Regen.3 

6 

0.9 

5 

4.9 

4 

1.7 

5 

2.5 

0 

0.0 

4 

1.5 

Total  caught 

679 

100.0 

103 

100.0 

224 

100.0 

204 

100.0 

201 

100.0 

260 

100.0 

Sex  ratio  M:F 

40:60 

51:50 

26:74 

61:39 

32:68 

37:63 

Size  ratio  S:M:L4 

1:25:74 

0:12:88 

16:9:75 

17:17:66 

25:5:70 

15:28:57 

1  Missing  =  crabs  with  one  or  more  scarred  stumps,  papillae,  or  limb  buds. 

:  Regenerating  =  crabs  possessing  one  or  more  functional  but  shortened  appendages. 

3  Miss.  +  Regen.  =  crabs  possessing  both  missing  and  regenerating  limbs. 

4  Size  ratio:  (S  <  61  mm  carapace  width.  M  =  61-1  10  mm  CW,  L  >  1 10  mm  CW)  for  all  crabs  (injured  and  intact). 
Sites  with  the  same  superscripted  letter  did  not  differ  significantly  in  total  autotomy  frequency. 


<  0.01;  Tables  II,  III).  In  the  Rhode  River  subestuary, 
limb  loss  frequency  was  significantly  higher  in  1986 
(25.0%)  than  in  1987-89  (STP  test,  3  df,  P  <  0.01:  Table 
II).  Levels  of  injury  in  the  latter  three  years  did  not  differ 
significantly.  The  frequency  of  limb  loss  from  1986  to 
1989  was  positively  correlated  with  estimated  annual 
mean  densities  of  crabs  based  on  trawl  net  collections 
(Hines  el  ai.  1990)  (Pearson's  correlation  coefficient,  r 
=  0.99,  P<  0.05). 

Frequencies  of  limb  loss  in  the  Rhode  River  in  spring 
(20.9%)  and  fall  ( 19.1%)  1989  did  not  differ  significantly 
from  the  overall  frequency  (17.8%)  for  the  entire  sampling 
season  (May  to  October).  This  yearly  value  is  used  for 
comparison  with  injury  levels  at  non-Rhode  River  sites 
in  spring  and  fall  1989.  The  frequency  of  limb  loss  in  the 
Rhode  River  subestuary  in  1989  was  identical  to  that  re- 
corded at  the  nearby  upper-mid  Chesapeake  Bay  site,  but 
much  lower  than  autotomy  frequencies  at  two  other  sites 
in  Chesapeake  Bay  (STP  test,  2  df,  P  <  0.01;  Tables  II, 
III).  Similarly,  the  frequency  of  limb  loss  in  the  Rhode 
River  in  1989  was  significantly  lower  than  springtime  in- 
jury levels  recorded  at  sites  in  South  Carolina  (3 1 .9%)  and 
Florida  (34.3%),  but  not  in  Alabama  (26.5%)  (STP  test; 
3  df;  P  <  0.01;  Tables  II,  III).  The  incidence  of  limb  au- 
totomy did  not  differ  significantly  among  Patuxent  River, 
lower-mid  Chesapeake  Bay,  South  Carolina,  Florida,  or 
Alabama  sites  (STP  test,  4  df,  P>  0.05),  despite  temporal 
and  geographic  differences  among  these  samples. 


Missing  versus  regenerating  limbs.  At  all  sites  in  the 
Chesapeake  Bay  and  in  two  of  four  years  in  the  Rhode 
River  (1986,  1989),  blue  crabs  were  missing  limbs  more 
often  than  they  were  regenerating  them  (G-tests,  1  df,  P 
<  0.05:  Tables  II,  III).  Blue  crabs  collected  from  Mobile 
Bay,  Alabama,  showed  the  opposite  trend,  missing  limbs 
less  often  than  they  were  regenerating  them  (G-test,  1  df, 
P  <  0.05;  Table  III).  No  significant  differences  in  fre- 
quencies of  individuals  missing  or  regenerating  append- 
ages were  observed  in  Indian  River,  Florida;  North  Inlet, 
South  Carolina;  or  the  Rhode  River,  Maryland,  in  1987 
and  1988.  Animals  simultaneously  missing  and  regener- 
ating limbs  were  rare  in  all  years  and  sites  (Tables  II,  III). 

Size  and  sex.  Of  all  variables  measured,  body  size  cor- 
related most  often  with  autotomy  frequencies  (Figs.  3,  4). 
In  the  Rhode  River,  large  animals  were  missing  or  regen- 
erating limbs  significantly  more  often  than  small  or  me- 
dium size  individuals  for  all  years  except  1988  (Fig.  3). 
Limb  loss  frequencies  did  not  differ  significantly  between 
small  and  medium  size  classes  in  any  year  (STP  tests,  2 
df.  Fig.  3).  Injury  frequencies  did  not  vary  significantly 
among  years  in  the  smallest  size  class,  but  between-year 
variation  in  injury  levels  was  observed  in  both  medium 
and  large  size  classes  (G-tests,  3  df,  P  <  0.05;  Fig.  5). 
Median  carapace  widths  of  all  injured  crabs  were  signif- 
icantly larger  than  those  of  all  intact  individuals  in  each 
year  (Mann- Whitney  U-tests,  P  <  0.001).  The  frequency 
of  autotomy  was  independent  of  sex  for  all  years  in  the 


422 


L    D.  SMITH  AND  A.  H.  MINES 


RHODE  RIVER,    MD 


40 

SEX     NS 

SIZE  »  SEX     NS 

35 

223                                   33fl 

30 

25 

189 

488 

20 

«  ; 

15 

10 
O 

2 

1987 

SIZE   ** 

SIZE  »  SEX     NS 


ui 
O 

IT 
LU 
0. 


SIZE  NS 
SEX  NS 
SIZE  x  SEX  NS 


SIZE     * 

SEX      NS 

SIZE  «  SEX     NS 


SIZE 

Figure  3.  Histograms  of  the  percentage  of  crabs  injured  in  Rhode 
River,  Maryland,  as  a  function  of  size  and  sex  for  each  year  (1986- 1989). 
S,  M,  and  L  represent  small  (carapace  width  <  61  mm),  medium  (61- 
1 10  mm),  and  large  (>  1 10  mm)  size  classes  of  crabs,  respectively.  Sample 
sizes  of  total  crabs  (i.e.,  injured  +  uninjured  animals)  in  each  category 
are  presented  above  each  bar.  Results  of  logistic  model  testing  for  as- 
sociation of  size,  sex,  and  the  interaction  of  size  and  sex  with  injury 
frequency  are  presented  for  each  year.  NS,  not  significant;  *,  P  <  0.05; 
**.  P<0.0\;***,  P<  0.001. 


Rhode  River  and  size  differences  were  the  same  for  both 
sexes  (Fig.  3). 

Outside  the  Rhode  River,  opposite  size-related  trends 
in  autotomy  frequencies  were  observed  at  upper-mid 
Chesapeake  Bay  and  South  Carolina  sites  (Fig.  4).  Patterns 
at  the  upper-mid  Chesapeake  Bay  site  resembled  those  of 
the  Rhode  River,  with  large  animals  showing  highest  in- 
cidences of  limb  loss.  In  contrast,  large  crabs  showed  the 
least  amount  of  limb  loss  in  North  Inlet,  South  Carolina 
and  males  were  injured  significantly  more  often  than  fe- 
males (STP  test,  P  <  0.05).  The  frequency  of  injury  was 
independent  of  size  and  sex  at  Patuxent  River,  lower-mid 
Chesapeake  Bay,  Indian  River,  and  Mobile  Bay  sites  (Fig. 
4).  At  non-Rhode  River  sites,  with  one  exception,  median 
carapace  widths  of  injured  and  intact  crabs  did  not  differ 
(Mann-Whitney  U-tests,  P  >  0.05).  At  the  upper-mid 
Chesapeake  Bay  site,  patterns  again  were  similar  to 
ones  observed  in  the  Rhode  River;  injured  crabs  were 
larger  than  uninjured  animals  (Mann-Whitney  U-test,  P 
<  0.002). 


Reproductive  maturity.  In  the  Rhode  River,  limb  loss 
and  reproductive  maturity  were  significantly  correlated 
for  females  in  1986  (male  reproductive  maturity  was  not 
measured)  and  for  both  sexes  in  1989.  In  1986,  mature 
female  crabs  showed  greater  frequency  of  limb  loss  (34%; 
n  =  132)  than  juvenile  females  (25%;  n  =  312)  (G-test, 
1  df,  P  =  0.05).  In  1989,  adults  of  both  sexes  (26%;  n 
=  324)  suffered  higher  levels  of  limb  loss  than  did  juveniles 
(10%;  n  =  359)  (logistic  regression,  4  df,  P  <  0.001).  No 
significant  differences  in  injury  were  observed  between 
juveniles  and  adults  in  the  Rhode  River  in  1987  and  1988, 
or  at  Chesapeake  Bay  and  southeastern  sites  with  the  ex- 
ception of  South  Carolina  (logistic  regression.  4  df,  P 


UPPER-MID   BAV,    MO 


1989 


SIZE  x  SEX      NS 

2J 

2 

34 

1 

NORTH    INLET,   SC 

SIZE  *• 
SEX   ** 

SIZE  »  SEX     NS 


PATUXENT    RIVER,    MD 


INDIAN    RIVER,   FL 

SIZE      NS 
SEX     NS 


a 

UJ 
H 


O 

oc 


SIZE  x  SEX     NS 

M 

1 

29 

/ 
,' 

,' 

22 

/ 

,' 
_•' 

1 

LOWER-MID   BAY,   VA 


MOBILE  BAY,  AL 


SIZE  NS 
SEX  NS 
SIZE  x  SEX  NS 


40 

SEX      NS 
SIZE  x  S 

EX     1 

S 

35 

7 

Z4 

3 

0 

30 

S 

1 

6 

25 

* 

20 

*• 

so 

15 

10 

^ 

5 

S 

j 

SIZE 

Figure  4.  Histograms  of  the  percentage  of  crabs  injured  at  sites  in 
the  Chesapeake  Bay,  South  Carolina,  Florida,  and  Alabama  as  a  function 
of  size  and  sex  in  1 989.  Size  categories  and  statistical  tests  are  as  described 
in  Figure  3.  In  the  Patuxent  River  site,  separate  tests  were  used  to  compare 
effects  of  ( 1 )  sex,  and  (2)  size  among  females.  Sample  sizes  were  too  low 
to  test  for  the  interaction  of  size  and  sex. 


AUTOTOMY   IN  BLUE  CRAB  POPULATIONS 


423 


20 
15 
10 


A 

139 


SMALL 


A 

129          A 
211 


113 


MEDIUM 


R       LARGE 

35]    559 


30 
25 
20 
15 


1986   1987    1988    1989 

YEAR 


Figure  5.  Between-year  comparisons  of  percentages  of  crabs  missing, 
regenerating,  and  both  missing  and  regenerating  limbs  in  the  Rhode 
River  by  size  from  1 986  to  1 989.  Years  with  the  same  superscripted 
letter  did  not  differ  significantly  in  total  autotomy  frequency  (STP  tests. 


<  0.006).  At  North  Inlet,  juvenile  crabs  showed  anoma- 
lously high  levels  of  limb  loss  (44%)  compared  to  adults 
(25%). 

Season.  The  percentage  of  injury  for  large  crabs  and 
for  combined  size  classes  in  the  Rhode  River  varied  sig- 
nificantly over  the  season  in  1987  and  1989  only  (G-tests, 
P  <  0.05,  Fig.  6).  In  these  years,  overall  levels  of  autotomy 
were  high  early  in  the  season,  declined  in  mid-summer 
(July-August),  increased  in  September,  and  dropped  again 
in  October.  These  late  season  declines  in  injury  level  were 
due  primarily  to  an  influx  of  smaller,  undamaged  crabs 
into  the  subestuary  (Hines  <:>/«/.,  1987a,  1 990).  Large  crabs 
continued  to  have  high  levels  of  damage  in  late  fall  (Fig. 
6).  No  significant  seasonal  trends  in  autotomy  frequency 
were  observed  for  small  or  medium  size  crabs  in  any  year. 

Subestuarine  location.  No  significant  differences  in  limb 
loss  were  found  among  sites  within  the  Rhode  River  sub- 


estuary  from  1986  to  1988.  In  contrast,  crabs  caught  at 
the  river  head  in  1989  were  missing  or  regenerating  limbs 
more  than  twice  as  often  (20%)  as  those  caught  at  the  river 
mouth  (9%)  (G-test,  1  df,  P  <  0.002). 

Molt  stage.  The  frequency  of  limb  loss  was  independent 
of  molt  stage  for  all  years  in  the  Rhode  River  and  at  all 
other  sites,  except  South  Carolina,  where  premolt  animals 
were  damaged  almost  twice  as  often  as  intermolt  animals 
(G-test,  2  df, /><  0.05). 

Patterns  of  autotomy 

Limb  number.  Single  limb  loss  was  the  most  common 
form  of  autotomy  for  all  sites  and  years  (Figs.  7,  8).  In 


LARGE 


80 

B 

21 

70 

' 

60 

*  *  * 

* 

50 
40 

115 

" 

NS 

toe 

2 

A             A 

NS 

A 

B 
B    A 

A      ,4 

Sfi 

82                        101 

' 

[7- 

Q        30 
UJ 

80 

A 

76    8S 

56 

22 
' 

]       B 
B   A 

1 

I  20 

A      96 

m 

24  r/lrx 

X     ^ 

'  SO    96 

Z      10 

11 

^~ 

• 

: 

}_ 

Y 

I 

£ 

H 

111 
o 

DC 

£                                            ALL  SIZES 

40 

* 

35 

NS 

238 

n-,                                    A 

NS 

A 

30 

as 

B              1!0    - 

42 

? 

b 

83                         B                  171 

25 

358          307 

A 

A 

17138 

71.            / 

/]A             r  y 

20 
15 

' 

28 

1    7J 

B 
A 

BF 

83 

" 

36 

^ 

57 

n 

B6 

' 

^ 

/I 

/20 

B    / 

B 

104 

10 

1! 

/ 

', 

5 

' 

/ 

/, 

JASON            J      J     A     S     0 

H 

J 

•" 

ASO         MJJASO 

1986                      1987 

1988               1989 

YEAR 

Figure  6.  Percentage  of  crabs  injured  (;.t'..  missing  or  regenerating 
at  least  one  limb)  by  month  in  the  Rhode  River,  Maryland  from  1986 
to  1989.  Large  crabs  (>l  10  mm  CW)  and  combined  size  classes  are 
presented.  Sample  sizes  and  results  of  2-way  contingency  tests  are  pre- 
sented above  each  bar.  NS  =  Not  significant;  *,  P  <  0.05;  **,  P  <  0.0 1, 
***,  P  <  0.00 1 .  For  each  year,  months  with  the  same  letter  were  not 
significantly  different  (STP  tests,  P  >  0.05). 


424 


L.  D.  SMITH  AND  A.  H.  MINES 


"nt. 


Figure  7.  Histogram  of  the  percentage  of  crabs  missing  or  regenerating 
1.  2,  3,  or  4  or  more  limbs  in  the  Rhode  River.  Maryland,  from  1986 
to  1989. 


the  Rhode  River,  1 1-17%  of  the  population  were  missing 
or  regenerating  a  single  limb,  while  injury  to  two  ap- 
pendages occurred  less  frequently  (3-6%).  Loss  of  three 
or  more  limbs  was  observed  in  less  than  2.5%  of  the  pop- 
ulation in  the  Rhode  River  for  any  given  year  (Fig.  7). 
The  maximum  number  of  limbs  missing  or  regenerating 
on  a  single  crab  was  six.  The  mean  number  of  limbs  lost 
ranged  from  1.3  to  1.6.  The  proportion  of  numbers  of 
limbs  (i.e..  1,  2,  3,  >4)  lost  among  crabs  in  the  Rhode 
River  did  not  differ  among  years  (G2  =  9.0,  9  df,  P  >  0. 1 ). 

The  relative  numbers  of  limbs  lost  also  did  not  differ 
among  blue  crabs  in  Alabama,  Florida,  upper-  or  lower- 
mid  Chesapeake  Bay  (Fig.  8).  In  North  Inlet,  South  Car- 
olina, single  limb  loss  was  proportionately  higher  than 
double  limb  loss  when  compared  to  other  sites  (STP  test, 
P  <  0.05).  In  Patuxent  River,  injury  to  two  limbs  ( 15.5%) 
occurred  nearly  as  often  as  single  autotomy  (19.4%).  The 
proportion  of  crabs  experiencing  single  versus  multiple 
limb  loss  did  not  differ  significantly  with  body  size  at  any 
site  (G-tests,  2  df,  P  >  0.05)  with  the  possible  exception 
of  the  Rhode  River  in  1986.  In  that  year,  only  13%  of  the 
injured  small  crabs  were  missing  or  regenerating  two  or 
more  limbs;  medium  (33%)  and  large  (36%)  crabs  showed 
considerably  higher  levels  of  multiple  autotomy  (G-test, 
2  df,  P  =  0.06). 

Although  comparatively  rare,  in  all  years  in  the  Rhode 
River  and  at  upper-  and  lower-mid  Chesapeake  Bay  sites, 
multiple  autotomy  occurred  more  often  than  would  be 


expected  based  on  a  binomial  distribution  in  which:  ( 1 ) 
the  probability  of  losing  any  one  limb  was  assumed  equal, 
and  (2)  limbs  were  independent  with  respect  to  damage 
(Table  IV).  In  contrast,  observed  and  expected  frequencies 
of  single  and  multiple  limb  loss  did  not  differ  significantly 
at  South  Carolina,  Florida,  and  Alabama  sites.  Observed 
and  expected  frequencies  of  limb  loss  were  marginally 
non-significant  (G-test,  2  df,  P  =  0.07)  in  the  Patuxent 
River. 

Limh  l\'iv  Chelipeds  were  the  most  common  limbs 
lost  in  all  populations  (8-33%)  (Figs.  9,  10).  Few  crabs 
were  missing  or  regenerating  the  paddle-shaped  fifth 
pereopod  ( 1-5%).  Different  limb  types  were  not  lost  with 
equal  frequency  at  any  site  or  in  any  year  (G-tests,  4  df, 
P  <  0.02).  The  proportions  of  injured  limb  types  did  not 
differ  in  the  Rhode  River  among  years  (G:  =  18.3.  12  df, 
P  >  0. 1 ).  Damage  to  chelipeds  was  disproportionately 
high  at  Florida,  lower-mid  Chesapeake  Bay.  and  Patuxent 
River  sites  when  compared  to  other  sites  (STP  tests,  20 
df,  P  <  0.05,  Fig.  10).  With  the  exception  of  the  South 
Carolina  site  and  the  Rhode  River  in  1988,  there  were  no 
differences  between  the  frequencies  of  right  and  left  limbs 
lost.  At  both  North  Inlet  in  1989  and  Rhode  River  in 
1988,  right  limbs  were  lost  more  often  than  left  limbs  (G- 
tests,  4df,  /)<0.05). 

C/ielipeil  morphology.  The  majority  (63-87%.)  of  crabs 
at  all  sites  and  in  all  years  possessed  a  right  crusher  cheliped 


Figure  8.  Histogram  of  the  percentage  of  crabs  missing  or  regenerating 
1,  2.  3,  or  4  or  more  limbs  in  the  upper-mid  Chesapeake  Bay  (UB); 
Patuxent  River.  Maryland  (PX);  lower-mid  Chesapeake  Bay  (LB):  North 
Inlet,  South  Carolina  (SC):  Indian  River,  Florida  (FL);  and  Mobile  Bay. 
Alabama  (AL)  in  1989. 


AUTOTOMY  IN  BLUE  CRAB  POPULATIONS 


425 


Table  IV 

n/K  nl  expected  versus  observed  frequencies  ot  mlacl  cmhs  and  those  missing  or  regenerating  I.  2.  J.  nr  4  or  more  limbs 
in  the  Rhode  River.  Maryland  from  1986  to  1989 

Rhode  River,  Maryland 


1986 


1987 


1988 


1989 


Iniurv 

status 

Obs. 

Exp. 

Obs. 

Exp 

Obs. 

Exp. 

Obs. 

Exp. 

Intact 

1050 

960 

505 

463 

536 

514 

569 

528 

-1  Limb 

234 

369 

75 

138 

86 

122 

78 

145 

-2  Limbs 

83 

64 

26 

18 

20 

13 

34 

18 

-3  Limbs 

21 

6 

12 

-) 

4 

1 

8 

1 

-  >4  Limbs 

12 

0.5 

4 

0.7 

3 

0.4 

3 

0.1 

G-test 

P 

<  0.001 

P  <  0.00  1 

P  <  0.005 

P<  0.001 

Expected  frequencies  were  generated  from  a  binomial  distribution  in  which  the  probability  of  loss  of  each  of  10  limbs  was  the  same.  Limbs  were 
assumed  to  be  lost  independently.  The  probability  of  losing  any  one  limb  =  #  limbs  lost  in  the  population/!  10  >  #  crabs  in  the  population).  The 
final  two  categories  (-3  and  -4  or  more  limbs)  were  pooled  for  analysis.  (G-tests.  2  df). 


and  a  left  cutter  cheliped  (Table  V).  The  frequency  of 
crabs  with  a  right  crusher/left  cutter  did  not  differ  among 
years  in  the  Rhode  River.  Frequencies  of  crabs  with  right 
crusher/left  cutter  morphology  in  the  upper-mid  Chesa- 
peake Bay  and  Rhode  River  in  1989,  however,  were  sig- 
nificantly higher  than  those  from  other  sites  in  that  year. 
Crabs  with  two  cutters  were  relatively  common  (7-21%); 
whereas,  left  crusher/right  cutter  morphological  patterns 
were  observed  less  frequently  (0.6-10%).  Crabs  possessing 
double  crushers  were  extremely  rare  (<1%). 


CRABS  WITH  INJURED  LIMB  TYPE 


Frequencies  of  crabs  bearing  right  crusher/left  cutter 
morphologies  decreased  as  size  increased  in  the  Rhode 
River  in  all  years  (Fig.  1 1 ).  The  frequency  of  female  crabs 
bearing  a  right  crusher/left  cutter  was  greater  than  males 
in  three  of  four  years  (P  =  0.06).  Sex  differences  in  the 
frequency  of  crusher/cutter  patterns  were  generally  con- 
sistent across  size  classes  (but  see  1986,  size  X  sex  inter- 
action). 


I  CRABS  WITH  INJURED  LIMB  TYPE 


Figure  9.  Histogram  of  the  percentage  of  crabs  missing  or  regenerating 
one  or  both  chelipeds.  1st,  2nd,  and  3rd  walking  legs  and  swimming  legs 
in  the  Rhode  River.  Maryland,  from  1986  to  1989. 


Figure  10.  Histogram  of  the  percentage  of  crabs  missing  or  regen- 
erating one  or  both  chelipeds,  1st.  2nd,  and  3rd  walking  legs  and  swim- 
ming legs  in  the  upper-mid  Chesapeake  Bay  (UB);  Patuxent  River, 
Maryland  (PX);  lower-mid  Chesapeake  Bay  (LB);  North  Inlet,  South 
Carolina  (SO:  Indian  River,  Florida  (FL);  and  Mobile  Bay.  Alabama 
(AL)  in  1989. 


426 


L.   D.  SMITH  AND  A.   H.   MINES 


Table  V 

Frequencies  and  percentages  of  crusher  and  cutler  chelipt'tl  murphi'logies  from  hlne  crabs  collected  in  the  Rhode  River  (1986-1989).  and  in  the 
iirifw-niii/  C  'hesapeake  Bay:  Patuxent  River:  lower-mid  Chesapeake  Bay:  North  Inlet,  SC:  Indian  River,  FL:  and  Mobile  Bay,  AL  in  1989 

Morphological  Patterns  of  Crab  Chelipeds 


Site  &  Year 

Right  crusher 
left  cutter 

Left  crusher 
nght  cutter 

Double 
cutters 

Double 
crushers 

Other 

Rhode  R.  86 

1109(79%)* 

61  (4%) 

148(11%) 

K.1%) 

81  (6%) 

Rhode  R.  87 

509  (82%)* 

18(3%) 

68  (IT':  ) 

0  (0%) 

27  (4%) 

Rhode  R.  88 

537  (83%)* 

4  (.6%) 

88(14%) 

0  (0%) 

20  (3%) 

Rhode  R.  89 

576(83%)*  a 

4  (.6%) 

93(13%) 

0  (0%) 

19(3%) 

Upper-Mid  CB  89 

593  (87%)    a 

20  (3%) 

44  (7%) 

2  (.3%) 

20  (3%) 

Patuxent  R.  89 

71  (69%)    b 

3  (3%) 

12  (12%) 

1  (1%) 

16(16%) 

Lower-Mid  CB  89 

163(73%)    b 

1  1  (5%) 

21  (9%) 

2(1%) 

27(12%) 

N.  Inlet,  SC  89 

129(63%)    b 

20(10%) 

42(21%) 

1  (.5%) 

12(6%) 

Indian  R.,  FL  89 

146(73%)    h 

1  1  (6%) 

20(10%) 

1  (.5%) 

23(11%) 

Mobile  B..  AL  89 

192(74%)    b 

20  (8%) 

37  (14%) 

2  (.8%) 

9  (4%) 

The  category  "other"  included  crabs  missing  one  cheliped  and  possessing  one  cutter  or  crabs  missing  both  chelipeds.  Comparisons  of  frequency 
of  crabs  with  a  right  crusher/left  cutter  for  4  years  in  the  Rhode  River  (STP  test,  3  df)  and  among  all  1989  sites  (STP  test,  6  df)  are  presented.  Sites 
with  the  same  symbol  or  letter  (to  denote  separate  tests)  are  not  significantly  different  (P  >  0.05). 


The  relationship  of  size  and  sex  to  cheliped  morphology 
was  less  consistent  at  sites  outside  the  Rhode  River.  The 
frequency  of  crabs  possessing  a  right  crusher/left  cutter 
did  not  vary  significantly  with  size  or  sex  at  any  Chesa- 
peake Bay  site  or  in  Mobile  Bay.  Low  sample  sizes  in  the 
Patuxent  River  prevented  testing  the  interaction  between 
size  and  sex.  Size  differences  were  recorded  in  North  Inlet 
and  Indian  River  (G-tests;  P  <  0.05).  At  North  Inlet,  large 
males  (47%;  n  =  84)  possessed  fewest  right  crushers.  Re- 
gardless of  size,  ca.  69%  of  female  blue  crabs  (n  =  82) 
possessed  a  right  crusher  and  left  cutter.  In  Indian  River, 
Florida,  the  incidence  of  right  crusher/left  cutters  was 
lower  in  large  individuals  of  both  sexes  when  compared 
to  small  size  classes. 


Discussion 


Causal  agents 


High  frequencies  of  limb  loss  recorded  over  broad  tem- 
poral and  geographic  scales  indicate  that  autotomy  is  an 
important  mechanism  for  survival  in  Callinectes  sapicius. 
Eighteen  to  25%  of  blue  crabs  surveyed  over  a  four  year 
period  in  the  Rhode  River,  Maryland,  and  19-39%  of 
blue  crabs  at  six  other  sites  along  the  eastern  coast  of  the 
United  States  in  1989  were  missing  or  regenerating  one 
or  more  limbs.  Autotomy  is  an  effective  escape  response 
to  predators  (Robinson  et  al.,  1 970;  Congdon  et  at. ,  1974; 
Medel  et  al.,  1988;  Smith,  1990a).  Variation  in  injury 
levels  in  populations  may  indicate  differential  predation 
pressure  (e.g.,  Shaffer,  1978;  Ballinger,  1979;  Schall  and 
Pianka,  1980;  McCallum  et  al..  1989)  or  predator  effi- 


ciency (Schoener,  1979;  Schoener  and  Schoener,  1980; 
Jaksic  and  Fuentes.  1980).  In  the  Rhode  River,  signifi- 
cantly higher  levels  of  limb  loss  were  recorded  in  blue 
crabs  in  1986  (25%)  than  in  three  subsequent  years  (18- 
19%).  If  partial  predation  is  responsible  for  autotomy 
(Smith,  1990a),  then  these  differences  indicate  either  in- 
creased predation  pressure,  decreased  predator  efficiency, 
or  both  during  1986. 

Several  lines  of  evidence  indicate  that  unsuccessful 
predation  by  conspecifics  may  be  the  principal  source  of 
nonlethal  injury  in  blue  crabs  in  the  Rhode  River.  Based 
on  trawl  catches  (Hines  et  al..  1987a,  1990),  the  Rhode 
River  subestuary  lacks  abundant  fish  predators  or  decapod 
species  capable  of  capturing  and  killing  medium-to-large, 
hard-shelled  blue  crabs.  American  eel  (Anguilla  rostrata) 
and  oyster  toadfish  (Opsanus  tail),  both  known  predators 
of  small  blue  crabs  (Wenner  and  Musick,  1975;  Wilson 
et  al..  1987),  occur  in  very  low  densities  in  the  subestuary 
(Hines  et  al..  1990).  Gut  analysis  (Laughlin,  1982:  Hines 
et  al..  1990)  and  experimental  work  (Peery,  1989;  Smith. 
1990a)  have  shown  that  cannibalism  is  an  important  cause 
of  mortality  in  blue  crabs.  A  long-term  study  in  Chesa- 
peake Bay  (Lipcius  and  Van  Engel,  1990)  suggested  den- 
sity-dependent regulation  of  blue  crab  populations  by 
conspecifics.  Increased  encounter  rates  between  conspe- 
cifics during  years  of  high  abundance  should  lead  to  in- 
creased levels  of  both  lethal  and  nonlethal  injury.  In  the 
present  survey,  the  frequency  of  limb  loss  was  positively 
correlated  with  annual  blue  crab  abundances  (correlation 
coefficient,  r  =  0.99)  in  the  Rhode  River.  Mean  abun- 
dances of  crabs  in  the  Rhode  River  between  1987  and 


AUTOTOMY  IN  BLUE  CRAB  POPULATIONS 


427 


RHODE    RIVER,   MD 


FEMALe 

MAI   I 


cc 

UJ 


cc 

LU 

I 


DC 

o 


I 

H 
g 


SIZE  •• 
SEX   • 

size  *  sex    p  <  o.oe 


SIZE  •* 
SEX  • 
SIZE  i  SEX  NS 


1988 

SIZE   • 

SEX    NS 

SIZE  x  SEX     NS 


1989 


SIZE  x  SEX     NS 

36 

69 

MB 

' 

43 

\ 

17 

92 

\\ 

ML  s         M        L 


S        M        L  S        M        L 


SIZE 


Figure  11.  Histograms  of  the  percentage  of  crabs  possessing  a  right 
crusher  and  left  cutter  in  the  Rhode  River.  Maryland,  as  a  function  of 
size  and  sex  for  each  year  ( 1 986-89).  S,  M,  and  L  represent  small  (carapace 
width  <  61  mm),  medium  (61-110  mm),  and  large  (>1IO  mm)  size 
classes  of  crabs,  respectively.  Sample  sizes  of  total  crabs  (i.e..  animals 
with  and  without  right  crusher-left  cutter  combinations)  in  each  category 
are  presented  above  each  bar.  Results  of  logistic  model  testing  for  the 
association  of  size,  sex,  and  their  interaction  with  the  frequency  of  crabs 
bearing  a  right  crusher-left  cutter  combination  are  presented  for  each 
year.  NS,  not  significant;  *,  P  <  0.05;  **,  P  <  0.01;  ***,  P  <  0.001. 


1989  (4-6  crabs/rrr)  were  significantly  lower  than  in  any 
of  the  five  previous  years  surveyed  (Hines  et  al..  1990). 
Injury  levels  in  the  Rhode  River  between  1987  and  1989 
remained  remarkably  constant,  which  suggests  temporal 
coupling  between  injury-causing  agents  and  victims. 

Factors  besides  partial  predation  (e.g.,  intraspecific 
competition,  fisheries)  could  contribute  to  observed  au- 
totomy  frequencies,  but  these  sources  are  probably  minor. 
In  most  brachyuran  crabs,  intraspecific  competitive  in- 
teractions are  highly  ritualized  and  do  not  usually  result 
in  limb  autotomy  (Hazlett,  1972;  Jachowski,  1974;  Hyatt 
and  Salmon,  1978).  In  Callinectes  sapidus,  instances  of 
limb  loss  were  rare  when  size-matched  males  competed 
for  a  sexually  receptive  female  in  small  arenas  (<1%; 
Smith,  1990a).  Limb  autotomy  can  occur  during  ecdysis 
in  blue  crabs,  but  such  instances  were  observed  infre- 
quently (Smith,  pers.  obs.).  Handling  by  fisheries  also 
could  contribute  to  injury  in  certain  size  classes  (e.g.. 


Kennelly  et  al..  1990).  Larger  sublegal-size  blue  crabs  (CW 
<  127  mm)  may  experience  limb  loss  before  being  culled. 
Smaller  crabs  generally  escape  uninjured  through  the 
mesh  of  crab  pots  (Smith,  pers.  obs.),  while  larger,  legal- 
sized  crabs  are  harvested. 

Autotomy  frequencies  were  not  biased  by  the  various 
gear  used  to  sample  blue  crabs.  Severely  injured  (and  po- 
tentially less  mobile)  animals  should  not  have  been  un- 
derrepresented  in  collections,  because  active  (otter  trawls, 
seines)  as  well  as  passive  (fish  weir,  crab  pots)  methods  of 
capture  were  used.  By  using  a  variety  of  collection  meth- 
ods, a  range  of  depths  and  habitats  in  the  subestuary  were 
sampled. 

Size  effects 

Positive  correlations  between  autotomy  frequency  and 
blue  crab  body  size  indicate  ontogenetic  differences  in 
both  repair  rates  and  susceptibility  to  predation.  The  av- 
erage percentage  of  injury  in  medium-  and  small-size  crabs 
for  four  years  in  the  Rhode  River  was  37%  and  58%  that 
of  large  crabs,  respectively.  Similar  size-related  trends  have 
been  observed  in  a  shore  crab,  Carcinus  maenas(Mc\ean, 
1976;  McVean  and  Findlay,  1979;  Sekkelsten,  1988). 
Predator  inefficiency  often  increases  with  increased  prey 
size  (Murtaugh,  1981;  Vermeij,  1982;Reaka,  1987;Peery, 
1989).  and  tethering  experiments  (Smith,  1990a)  have 
demonstrated  that  larger  blue  crabs  suffered  appendage 
loss  proportionately  more  often  than  mortality  compared 
to  smaller  crabs.  In  the  present  survey,  median  carapace 
widths  of  injured  crabs  in  the  Rhode  River  were  greater 
than  those  of  uninjured  individuals.  Evidence  of  limb  loss 
will  also  remain  for  longer  periods  in  large  than  small 
animals.  Limb  regeneration  requires  molting,  and  blue 
crab  molting  frequency  declines  as  size  increases  (Leffler, 
1972;  Smith,  1990b).  In  St.  Johns  River,  Florida,  the  av- 
erage length  of  the  molt  interval  (ca.  40  days)  for  large 
(>1 10  mm  CW)  crabs  was  2.5  times  that  of  small  crabs 
(16  days;  20-59  mm  CW)  and  1.5  times  that  of  medium 
crabs  (27  days;  60-1 10  mm  CW)  (Tagatz,  1968;  see  also 
Smith,  1990b).  Based  on  these  molt  intervals,  estimated 
daily  injury  rates  for  crabs  in  a  given  size  class  (i.e.,  % 
injury/molt  interval)  over  four  years  in  the  Rhode  River 
were  similar  (ca.  0.74%/day  for  small  crabs,  0.64%/day 
for  medium  crabs,  and  0.69%/day  for  large  crabs).  Al- 
though small  crabs  are  more  vulnerable  to  fatal  attack 
from  predators  than  medium  or  large  crabs  (Smith, 
1990a),  they  will  regenerate  missing  limbs  more  quickly 
after  nonlethal  injury.  In  female  blue  crabs,  molting  ceases 
when  sexual  maturity  is  reached  (Millikin  and  Williams, 
1984),  so  subsequent  injuries  accumulate. 

Temporal  variation 

The  lack  of  significant  monthly  variation  in  injury  for 
small-  and  medium-size  blue  crabs  within  years  in  Rhode 


428 


L.   D    SMITH   AND  A.   H.  MINES 


River  indicates  that  predator  efficiency  remained  season- 
ally consistent  for  both  size  classes.  Significant  between- 
year  differences  among  medium-size  crabs,  however,  sug- 
gests that  as  annual  predation  levels  change,  medium-size 
animals  may  experience  greater  variability  in  survival  than 
smaller  animals.  Injury  levels  in  large  crabs  exhibited  both 
significant  within-  and  between-year  variability.  Higher 
frequencies  of  limb  loss  in  the  large  size  class  late  in  the 
season  (September-October)  could  have  resulted  from  a 
combination  of  factors:  ( 1 )  slower  repair  rates  as  average 
sizes  increased  over  the  summer;  (2)  decreasing  molting 
frequency  as  water  temperature  declined  (Leffler,  1972); 
and  (3)  increased  levels  of  cannibalism  as  bivalve  prey 
(e.g..  Alya  arenaria,  Afacoma  balthica)  became  scarce 
(Hines  ct  a/.,  1990).  High  frequencies  of  limb  loss  seen  at 
the  beginning  of  each  season  may  be  a  carryover  from  the 
previous  fall.  Because  molt  frequency  declines  over  winter, 
regeneration  is  delayed. 

Sex 

Male  and  female  blue  crabs,  regardless  of  stage  of  sexual 
maturity,  appeared  equally  vulnerable  to  injury  in  the 
Rhode  River.  This  is  consistent  with  observations  in  Car- 
cinus  maenas  (McVean  and  Findlay,  1979)  and  Cancer 
magister  (Shirley  and  Shirley,  1988).  Given  that  adult 
male  blue  crabs  continue  to  molt,  it  is  surprising  that 
injury  frequencies  in  mature  females  were  not  propor- 
tionately higher.  It  is  possible  that:  ( 1 )  large  adult  males 
are  molting  so  infrequently  that  they  rarely  restore  limb 
symmetry.  (2)  behavioral  differences  are  making  mature 
females  less  prone  to  injury  (but  see  Smith,  1990a);  or  (3) 
females  are  migrating  to  spawning  areas  in  southern 
Chesapeake  Bay,  so  their  injuries  are  not  observed  in  the 
Rhode  River. 

Spatial  variation 

Injury  frequencies  did  not  vary  spatially  within  the 
Rhode  River  subestuary  in  three  of  four  years.  Hines  et 
at.  (1987a)  have  shown  that  blue  crabs  enter  the  Rhode 
River  each  spring  and  fall  where  they  grow  to  maturity. 
Male  crabs  forage  throughout  the  subestuary  and  use 
Muddy  Creek  as  a  molting  habitat.  These  movement  pat- 
terns may  explain  why  observed  injury  levels  are  homo- 
geneous across  sites. 

Significant  differences  among  autotomy  frequencies  in 
the  Rhode  River  region,  other  sites  in  the  Chesapeake 
Bay,  and  southeastern  United  States  indicate  that  these 
regions  differ  in  the  type,  degree,  or  efficiency  of  injury- 
causing  agents.  Injury  levels  recorded  in  the  Rhode  River 
and  upper-mid  Chesapeake  Bay  in  1989  were  markedly 
lower  than  at  any  other  site  (except  Alabama)  for  that 
year.  Higher  frequencies  of  limb  loss  and  regeneration 


outside  the  Rhode  River  cannot  be  attributed  to  differ- 
ences in  sex  ratio  or  size  distributions  among  sites,  because 
the  elevated  injury  levels  were  maintained  for  most  cat- 
egories of  size  and  sex.  The  relatively  low  salinities  and 
shallow  depths  found  in  the  Rhode  River  may  limit  the 
abundances  and  diversity  of  predators  so  that  the  subes- 
tuary serves  as  a  refuge.  Qualitative  observations  of  trawl 
catches  at  the  Patuxent  River,  lower-mid  Chesapeake  Bay. 
and  Alabama  sites  showed  higher  diversity  and  abun- 
dances of  large,  known  crab  predators  (e.g.,  striped  bass. 
Morone  saxatilis;  oyster  toadfish,  Opsanus  tan:  white  cat- 
fish, Ictalurus  catus,  Millikin  and  Williams,  1984)  than 
were  found  in  the  Rhode  River  (Hines  et  ai.  1990). 

Surprisingly,  no  significant  differences  in  injury  fre- 
quency existed  among  populations  from  the  Patuxent 
River,  Maryland  south  to  Mobile  Bay.  Alabama,  even 
though  these  populations  spanned  two  biogeographic 
provinces  (cold-temperate  North  Atlantic  and  warm- 
temperate  Northwest  Atlantic:  Vermeij,  1978),  were  sam- 
pled in  different  seasons,  and  were  subjected  to  different 
suites  of  predators.  These  data  contrast  with  studies  show- 
ing increased  predation  pressure  at  lower  latitudes  (Bert- 
ness  et  at.,  1981;  Vermeij  et  ai,  1980;  Heck  and  Wilson, 
1987). 

Patterns  of  autotomy 

The  consistency  of  limb  loss  pattern  observed  in  this 
study  is  probably  due  to  limb  function  and  the  behavioral 
response  to  the  injury-causing  agent.  Chelipeds  were  lost 
most  often,  followed  by  first  walking  legs.  Similar  patterns 
have  been  observed  in  other  brachyuran  crabs  (e.g..  Car- 
cinus  maenas,  McVean,  1976;  McVean  and  Findlay. 
1979;  Cancer  magister,  Durkin  et  al.,  1984;  Shirley  and 
Shirley.  1988).  Crabs  respond  to  threats  from  predators 
or  competitors  with  outstretched  claws  (Schone,  1968: 
Robinson  et  al..  1970;  Jachowski,  1974;  Vannini,  1980) 
making  anterior  limbs  particularly  vulnerable  to  injury. 
Strikes  from  behind  may  often  prove  fatal,  so  fewer  crabs 
will  be  found  missing  swimming  legs.  Additionally,  the 
autotomy  response  in  swimming  legs  is  greatly  reduced 
in  larger  crabs;  even  severe  damage  to  these  limbs  often 
would  not  result  in  autotomy  (Smith,  pers.  obs.).  Small 
and  medium-sized  crabs,  however,  autotomize  all  limb 
types  readily.  Escape  responses  by  blue  crabs  showed  no 
consistent  directionality  (Smith.  1990a),  and  the  sym- 
metry of  limb  loss  suggests  that  the  injury-causing  agent 
is  striking  randomly.  Similarity  in  injury  frequency  be- 
tween right  and  left  sides  has  also  been  observed  in 
Dungeness  crabs.  Cancer  magister  (Durkin  et  al..  1984). 

Multiple  autotomies  could  be  caused  either  by  single 
events  damaging  more  than  one  leg  or  by  cumulative 
damage  from  independent  events.  While  single  limb  loss 


AUTOTOMY  IN  BLUE  CRAB  POPULATIONS 


429 


was  most  common  at  all  sites  and  in  all  years  in  Callinectes 
sapidus  (also  in  Carcinus  maenas,  McVean  and  Findlay, 
1979:  Cancer  magister,  Shirley  and  Shirley,  1988),  mul- 
tiple limb  loss  was  more  frequent  than  chance  predicts. 
McVean  (1976)  interpreted  a  similar  pattern  in  C.  maenas 
to  indicate  that  injured  animals  are  more  susceptible  to 
attack  than  intact  individuals.  Tethering  studies  in  C'. 
sapidus  suggest  that  multiple  limb  loss  occurs  in  a  single 
attack  event  (Smith,  1990a).  The  percentage  of  animals 
simultaneously  missing  and  regenerating  limbs  was  rare 
(ca.  1%)  in  all  years  in  the  Rhode  River,  indicating  that 
previous  limb  loss,  in  most  instances,  does  not  make  an 
animal  more  vulnerable  to  future  attacks. 

Cheliped  regeneration 

Substantial  percentages  of  regenerating  chelipeds  were 
observed  in  all  populations,  which  suggests  that,  despite 
their  importance  (e.g..  defense,  foraging),  crabs  could 
compensate  temporarily  for  their  loss.  In  many  crustacean 
taxa,  loss  of  the  major  claw  results  in  the  transformation 
of  the  opposing  minor  claw  into  a  major  claw  over  several 
molts  (Hamilton  el  ai.  1976).  The  autotomized  limb  is 
simultaneously  replaced  by  a  minor  claw.  In  blue  crabs, 
transformation  can  be  incomplete  even  after  three  molts 
(Smith,  1990b);  consequently,  those  crabs  losing  a  right 
crusher  claw  bear  symmetrical,  double  cutters  following 
regeneration.  Presence  of  a  left  crusher/ right  cutter,  double 
cutters,  or  double  crushers  is  evidence  of  previous  limb 
loss.  In  the  Rhode  River,  frequencies  of  animals  bearing 
a  right  crusher/left  cutter  generally  declined  as  size  in- 
creased. Up  to  25%  of  large  crabs  (e.g.,  1986;  Fig.  1 1) 
showed  evidence  of  having  lost  a  right  crusher  during  their 
lifetime,  which  suggests  that  survival  following  loss  of  a 
right  crusher  was  high.  Frequencies  of  these  atypical  claw 
morphologies  were  even  higher  in  South  Carolina  (32%) 
and  Florida  (35%.).  Interestingly,  the  percentage  of  male 
crabs  bearing  a  right  crusher  and  left  cutter  was  lower 
than  in  females  in  three  out  of  four  years  in  the  Rhode 
River,  which  suggests  that  males  were  suffering  greater 
incidence  of  cheliped  injury  during  their  lifetime. 

Conclusions 

By  examining  the  frequency  of  injury  over  both  tem- 
poral and  geographic  scales,  our  study  provides  the  most 
complete  analysis  to  date  on  autotomy  in  any  species. 
The  magnitude  of  this  data  set  allows  inferences  about 
causal  agents  of  autotomy  and  about  the  impact  of  au- 
totomy on  blue  crab  survival  following  attack.  Four  years 
of  autotomy  data  in  the  Rhode  River,  Maryland,  provide 
evidence  that:  ( 1 )  the  frequency  of  nonlethal  injury  in  the 
population  is  positively  correlated  with  density  and  is 
probably  due  to  unsuccessful  conspecific  predation;  (2) 


the  rate  of  autotomy  is  similar  over  the  lifespan  of  the 
individual,  but  differences  in  molting  rate  and  predator 
efficiency  result  in  higher  injury  levels  in  larger  animals; 
(3)  chances  of  survival  subsequent  to  single  or  double  limb 
loss  are  good;  and  (4)  lower  frequencies  of  autotomy  in 
the  Rhode  River  compared  to  other  sites  indicate  geo- 
graphic differences  in  the  intensity  or  efficiency  of  injury- 
causing  agents.  The  high  incidence  of  limb  loss  in  all  age 
groups,  and  in  both  sexes  over  broad  temporal  and  geo- 
graphic scales,  indicates  that  autotomy  is  an  important 
adaptation  for  avoiding  predation. 

Acknowledgments 

We  wish  to  thank  the  many  individuals  who  have  as- 
sisted us  in  this  study.  We  extend  our  sincere  appreciation 
to  Dr.  D.  Allen,  J.  Dimitry.  J.  Dindo.  A.  Fanning,  P. 
Geer.  M.  Haddon.  J.  Harding.  L.  Johnson,  W.  Lee,  D. 
Lello,  Dr.  S.  Morgan.  D.  Palmer.  H.  Reichardt,  Dr.  M. 
Rice,  R.  Speers,  G.  Tritaik,  and  L.  Wiechert.  for  their 
assistance  in  collecting  and  measuring  animals;  and  Dr. 
E.  Russek-Cohen  for  her  statistical  advice.  This  manu- 
script has  benefited  from  critical  comments  by  Dr.  J. 
Dineen,  D.  Lello.  Dr.  M.  Raupp.  Dr.  M.  Reaka,  Dr.  G. 
Vermeij,  and  two  anonymous  reviewers.  We  are  grateful 
to  them  all.  The  project  has  been  supported  by  the  Lerner- 
Gray  Fund;  Sigma  Xi  Grants-in-Aid  of  Research,  the  De- 
partment of  Zoology's  Chesapeake  Bay  Fund  at  the  Uni- 
versity of  Maryland,  and  a  Smithsonian  Predoctoral  Fel- 
lowship, all  to  L.  D.  Smith,  and  grants  to  A.  H.  Hines 
from  the  Smithsonian  Environmental  Sciences  Program, 
Smithsonian  Scholarly  Studies  Program,  and  the  National 
Science  Foundation  OCE-8700414.  This  paper  was  sub- 
mitted in  partial  fulfillment  of  a  Doctor  of  Philosophy  at 
the  University  of  Maryland. 

Literature  Cited 

Ballinger,  R.  E.  1979.     Intraspecific  variation  in  demography  and  life 

history  of  the  lizard,  Sceloporus  jarrovi,  along  an  altitudinal  gradient 

in  southeastern  Arizona.  Ecology  60:  90 1-909. 
Bennett,  D.  B.  1973.     The  effect  of  limb  loss  and  regeneration  on  the 

growth  of  the  edible  crab.  Cancer  pagimis  L.  J.  Exp.  Mar.  Biol.  Ecol. 

13:  45-53. 
Bertness,  M.  D.,  S.  D.  Garrity,  and  S.  C.  Levings.  1981.     Predation 

pressure  and  gastropod  foraging:  a  tropical-temperate  comparison. 

Evolution  35:  995-1007. 
Berzins,  I.  K.,  and  R.  L.  Caldwell.  1983.     The  effect  of  injury  on  the 

agonistic  behavior  of  the  stomatopod.  Gonodactylus  bredini  (Man- 
ning). Mar.  Behav.  Physiol.  10:  83-96. 
Congdon,  J.  D.,  L.  J.  Vitt,  and  \V.  \V.  King.  1974.     Geckos:  adaptive 

significance  and  energetics  ol  tail  autotomy.  Science  184:  1379-1380. 
Conover,  M.  R.,  and  D.  E.  Miller.  1978.  The  importance  of  the  large 

chela  in  the  territorial  and  pairing  behaviour  of  the  snapping  shrimp. 

Alpheus  heterochaelis.  Mar.  Behav.  Physiol.  5:  185-192. 
Cox,  D.  R.  1970.    Analysis  of  Binary  Data  Metheun,  London.  142  pp. 


430 


L.  D.  SMITH  AND  A.  H.  MINES 


Dial,  B.  E.,  and  L.  C.  Fitzpatrick.  1983.  Lizard  tail  autotomy:  function 
and  energetics  of  postautotomy  tail  movement  in  Scince/la  laleralis. 
Science  219:  391-393. 

Dial,  B.  E.,  and  L.  C.  Fitzpatrick.  1984.  Predator  escape  success  in 
tailed  versus  tailless  Scincella  lalera/is  (Sauria:Scincidae).  Anim.  Be- 
ha\:  32:  301-302. 

Durkin,  J.  T.,  K.  D.  Buchanan,  and  T.  H.  Blahm.  1984.  Dungeness 
crab  leg  loss  in  the  Columbia  River  estuary.  Mar.  Fish.  Rev.  46:  22- 
24. 

Edwards,  J.  S.  1972.     Limb  loss  and  regeneration  in  two  crabs:  the  king 
crab  Paralithodcs  camtschatica  and  the  tanner  crab  Chionoecetes 
bairdi.  Ada  Zoo/.  53:  105-1 12. 
Fox,  L.  R.  1975.     Cannibalism  in  natural  populations.  Ann.  Rev.  Ecol. 

Sysl.  6:87-106. 

Hamilton,  P.  V.  1976.     Cheliped  laterally  in  Callinectes  sapidus  (Crus- 
tacea: Portunidae).  Biol.  Bull.  150:  393-401. 
Harris,  R.  N.  1989.     Nonlethal  injury  to  organisms  as  a  mechanism  of 

population  regulation.  Am  Nat   134:  835-847. 
Hazlett,  B.  A.  1972.     Responses  to  agonistic  postures  by  the  spider  crab 

Microphrvs  bicormiltis.  Mar.  Beliav.  Physiol.  1:  85-92. 
Heck,  K.  L.,  Jr.,  and  K.  A.  Wilson.  1987.     Predation  rates  on  decapod 
crustaceans  in  latitudinally  separated  seagrass  communities:  a  study 
of  spatial  and  temporal  variation  using  tethering  techniques.  J.  Exp. 
Mar.  Biol.  Ecol.  107:  87-100. 

Hines,  A.  H.,  R.  N.  Lipcius,  and  A.  M.  Haddon.  1987a.  Population 
dynamics  and  habitat  partitioning  by  size,  sex.  and  molt  stage  of  blue 
crabs  Callinectes  sapidus  in  a  subestuary  of  central  Chesapeake  Bay. 
Mar.  Ecol.  Prog.  Ser  36:  55-64. 

Hines,  A.  H.,  P.  J.  Haddon,  J.  J.  Miklas,  L.  A.  VViechert,  and  A.  M. 
Haddon.  1987b.  Estuarine  invertebrates  and  fish:  sampling  design 
and  constraints  for  long-term  measurements  of  population  dynamics. 
Pp.  140-164  in  New  Approaches  to  Monitoring  Aquatic  Ecosystems. 
ASTM  STP  940.  T.  P.  Boyle,  ed.  American  Society  for  Testing  and 
Materials.  Philadelphia. 

Hines,  A.  H.,  A.  M.  Haddon,  and  L.  A.  Wiechert.  1990.     Guild  structure 
and  foraging  impact  of  blue  crabs  and  epibenthic  fish  in  a  subestuary 
of  Chesapeake  Bay.  Mar.  Ecol.  Prog.  Ser.  67:  105-126. 
Hyatt,  G.  VV.,  and  M.  Salmon.  1978.     Combat  in  the  fiddler  crabs  L'ca 
pugi/ator  and  L'.  pugnax:  a  quantitative  analysis.  Behaviour  65:  182- 
211. 
Jachowski,  R.  L.  1974.     Agonistic  behaviour  of  the  blue  crab.  Callinectes 

sapidus  Rathbun.  Behaviour  50:  232-253. 
Jaeger,  R.  G.  1981.     Dear  enemy  recognition  and  the  costs  of  aggression 

between  salamanders.  Am  Nat.  117:  962-974. 
Jaksic,  F.  M.,  and  E.  R.  Fuentes.  1980.     Correlates  of  tail  losses  in 

twelve  species  of  Liolaemus  lizards.  J.  Herpetcil.  14:  137-141. 
Johnson,  P.  T.  1980.     Histology  ot  the  Blue  Crab.  Callinectes  sapidus. 
A  Model  for  the  Decapoda.  Praeger  Sci.  Publ.  Co.,  New  York.  440 
pp. 

Kennelly,  S.  J.,  D.  Watkins,  and  J.  R.  Craig.  1990.  Mortality  of  dis- 
carded spanner  crabs  Ranina  ranina  (Linnaeus)  in  a  tangle-net  fish- 
ery— laboratory  and  field  experiments.  J.  E.\p.  Mar.  Biol.  Ecol.  140: 
39-48. 

Kuris,  A.  M.,  and  M.  Mager.  1975.     Effect  of  limb  regeneration  on  size 
increase  at  molt  of  the  shore  crabs  Hemigrapsus  oregonensis  and 
Pachygrapsus  crassipes.  J.  Exp.  Zoo/.  193:  353-360. 
Kurihara,  Y.,  and  K.  Okamoto.  1987.     Cannibalism  in  a  grapsid  crab, 

Hemigrapsus penicillatus.  Mar.  Ecol.  Prog.  Ser  41:  123-127. 
l.aughlin,  R.  A.  1982.     Feeding  habits  of  the  blue  crab.  Callinectes  sap- 
ulus  Rathbun.  in  the  Apalachicola  estuary.  Florida.  Bull.  Mar.  Sci. 
32:  807-822. 

Leffler,  C.  VV.  1972.  Some  effects  of  temperature  on  the  growth  and 
metabolic  rate  of  juvenile  blue  crabs.  Callinectes  sapidus.  in  the  lab- 
oratory. Mar.  Ecol.  Prog.  Ser.  41:  123-127. 


Lipcius,  R.  M.,  and  W.  A.  Van  Engel.  1990.     Blue  crab  population 

dynamics  in  Chesapeake  Bay:  variation  in  abundance  (York  River. 

1972-1989)  and  stock-recruit  functions.  Bull.  Mar  Sci.  46:  180- 

194. 

Maiorana,  V.  C.  1977.     Tail  autotomy.  functional  conflicts  and  their 

resolution  by  a  salamander.  Nature  265:  533-535. 
McCallum,  H.  L,  R.  Endcan.  and  A.  M.  Cameron.  1989.     Sublethal 
damage  to  Acanthaster  planet  as  an  index  of  predation  pressure.  Mar. 
Ecol.  Prog.  Ser.  56:  29-36. 
McVean,  A.  1976.     The  incidence  of  autotomy  in  Carcinus  nnicmis 

(L.).  /  Exp.  Mar  Biol  Ecol.  24:  177-187. 
McVean,  A.  1982.     Autotomy.  Pp.  107-132  in  The  Biology  of  Crustacea, 

Vol.  4,  D.  E.  Bliss,  ed.  Academic  Press,  Inc.,  New  York. 
McVean,  A.,  and  I.  Findlay.  1979.     The  incidence  of  autotomy  in  an 
estuarine  population  of  the  crab  Carcinus  maenas.  J.  Mar.  Biol.  Assoc. 
U.K.  59:341-354. 

Medel,  R.  G.,  J.  E.  Jimenez,  S.  F.  Fox,  and  F.  M.  Jaksic. 
1988.  Experimental  evidence  that  high  population  frequencies  of 
lizard  tail  autotomy  indicate  inefficient  predation.  Oikos  53:  321- 
324. 

Millikin,  M.  R.,  and  A.  B.  Williams.  1984.     Synopsis  of  biological  data 
on  the  blue  crab.  Callinecles  sapidus  Rathbun.  NOA.4  Technical  Re- 
port NMFS  1.  FAO  Fisheries  Synopsis  No.  138. 
Murtaugh,  P.  A.  1981.     Inferring  properties  of  mysid  predation  from 

injuries  to  Daphnia.  Limnol.  Oceanogr.  26:  81 1-821. 
Needham,  A.  E.  1953.     The  incidence  and  adaptive  value  of  autotomy 
and  of  regeneration  in  Crustacea.  Proc.  Zool.  Soc.  London  123:  111- 
122. 

Peery,  C.  A.  1989.     Cannibalism  experiments  with  the  blue  crab  (Cal- 
linectes sapidus  Rathbun):  potential  effects  of  size  and  abundance. 
Master's  thesis.  College  of  William  and  Mary,  Williamsburg,  Virginia. 
Polls,  G.  A.  1981.     The  evolution  and  dynamics  of  intraspecific  pre- 
dation. Ann.  Rev.  Ecol.  Syst.  12:  225-251. 

Reaka,  M.  L.  1987.     Adult-juvenile  interactions  in  benthic  reef  crus- 
taceans. Bull.  Mar.  Sci.  41:  108-134. 
Robinson,  M.  H.,  L.  G.  Abele,  and  B.  Robinson.  1970.     Attack  autotomy: 

a  defense  against  predators.  Science  169:  300-301. 
SAS  Institute.  1985.     SAS  User's  Guide:  Statistics.  Version  5  Edition. 

SAS  Institute  Inc.,  Cary,  North  Carolina.  956  pp. 
Schall,  J.  J.,  and  E.  R.  Pianka.  1980.     Evolution  of  escape  behavior 

diversity.  Am  Nat.  155:  551-566. 

Schoener,  T.  \\.  1979.  Inferring  the  properties  of  predation  and  other 
injury-producing  agents  from  injury  frequencies.  Ecology  60:  1 1 10- 
1115. 

Schoener,  T.  VV.,  and  A.  Schoener.  1980.  Ecological  and  demographic 
correlates  of  injury  rates  in  some  Bahamian  Anolis  lizards.  Copeia 
1980:  839-850. 

Schone,  H.  1968.     Agonistic  and  sexual  display  in  aquatic  and  semi- 
terrestrial  brachyuran  crabs.  Am.  Zool.  8:  641-654. 
Sekkelsten,  G.  I.  1988.     Effect  of  handicap  on  mating  success  in  male 

shore  crabs  Carcinus  maenas.  Oikos  51:  131-134. 
Shaffer,  H.  B.  1 978.     Relative  predation  pressure  on  salamanders  (Cau- 
data:Plethodontidae)  along  an  altitudmal  transect  in  Guatemala. 
Copeia  1978:  268-272. 

Shirley,  S.  M.,  and  T.  C.  Shirley.  1988.  Appendage  injury  in  dungeness 
crabs.  Cancer  magisler  in  southeastern  Alaska.  Fish.  Bull.  86:  156- 
160. 

Simonson,  J.  L.  1985.  Reversal  of  handedness.  growth,  and  claw  stri- 
dulatory  patterns  in  the  stone  crab  Menippe  mercenaria  (Say)  (Crus- 
tacea:Xanthidae).  J.  Crust.  Biol.  5:  281-293. 

Simonson,  J.  L.,  and  P.  Steele.  1981 .  Cheliped  asymmetry  in  the  stone 
crab,  Menippe  mercenaria.  with  notes  on  claw  reversal  and  regen- 
eration. Northeast  (.iu/j'Sci,  5:  21-30. 


AUTOTOMY  IN  BLUE  CRAB  POPULATIONS 


431 


Slater,  A.,  and  J.  M.  Lawrence.  1980.     The  effect  of  arm  loss  on  feeding 

and  growth  rates  in  Luulia  clathrata  Fla.  Sci.  43:  16. 
Smith,  L.  D.  1990a.     The  frequency  and  ecological  consequences  of 

limb  autotomy  in  the  blue  crab,  Callinectes  sapidtis  Rathbun.  Ph.D. 

Dissertation.  University  of  Maryland,  College  Park.  259  pp. 
Smith,  L.  D.  1990b.     Patterns  of  limb  loss  in  the  blue  crab,  Callmecles 

sapidiis  Rathbun.  and  the  effects  of  autotomy  on  growth.  Bull.  Mar 

Sci.  46:  23-36. 
Sokal,  R.  R.,  and  F.  J.  Rohlf.  1981.     Biometry.  W.  H.  Freeman.  New 

York.  859  pp. 
Stevens,  B.  G.,  D.  A.  Armstrong,  and  R.  Cusimano.  1982.     Feeding 

habits  of  the  Dungeness  crab  Cancer  magislcr  as  determined  by  the 

index  of  relative  importance.  Mar.  Bioi  72:  135-145. 
Sullivan,  J.  R.  1979.     The  stone  crab,  Menippe  mercenaria.  in  the 

southwest  Florida  fishery.  Ft.  Mar.  Res.  Publ.  No.  36. 
Tagatz,  M.  E.  1968.     Growth  of  juvenile  blue  crabs,  ( 'allmectes  sapidiis 

Rathbun,  in  the  St.  Johns  River.  Florida.  Fish  Bull  67:  2X1-288. 
Van  Engel,  W .  A.  1958.     The  blue  crab  and  its  fishery  in  the  Chesapeake 

Bay.  Part  I — Reproduction,  early  development,  growth  and  migra- 
tion. Cinnmcr.  Fish.  Rev.  20:  6-17. 
Van  Engel,  W .  A.  1990.     Development  of  the  reproductively  functional 

form  in  the  male  blue  crab,  Callinectes  sapidiis.  Bull.  Mar  Sci  46: 

13-22. 
Vannini,  M.  1980.     Notes  on  the  behaviour  ofOcypoile  mien  Kingsley 

(Crustacea,  Brachvura).  Mar  Behav.  Physiol.  1:  171-183. 


Vermeij,  G.  J.  1976.     Interoceanic  differences  in  vulnerability  of  shelled 

prey  to  crab  predation.  Nature  260:  135-136. 
Vermeij,  G.  J.  1977.     The  Mesozoic  marine  revolution:  evidence  from 

snails,  predators  and  grazers.  Paleobiology  3:  243-258. 
Vermeij,  G.  J.  1978.     Bioxcograpliy  and  Adaptation:  Patterns  of  Marine 

Lite  Harvard  University  Press,  Cambridge,  MA.  332  pp. 
Vermeij,  G.  J.  1982.     Unsuccessful  predation  and  evolution.  Am.  Nat. 

120:  701-720. 
Vermeij,  G.  J.  1983.     Shell-breaking  predation  through  time.  Pp.  649- 

669  in  Biolic  Interactions  in  Recent  and  Fossil  Benthic  Communities. 

M.  J.  S.  Tevesz  and  P.  L.  McCall.  eds.  Plenum,  New  York. 
Vermeij,  G.  J.,  E.  Zipser,  and  E.  C.  Dudley.  1980.     Predation  in  time 

and  space:  peeling  and  drilling  in  terebrid  gastropods.  Paleobiology 

6:  352-364. 
Vitt,  L.  J.,  J.  D.  Congdon,  and  N.  A.  Dickson.  1977.     Adaptive  strategies 

and  energetics  of  tail  autotomy  in  lizards.  Ecology  58:  326-337. 
VV'enner,  C.  A.,  and  J.  A.  Musick.  1975.     Food  habits  and  seasonal 

abundance  of  the  American  eel,  Anguilla  roslrata.  from  the  lower 

Chesapeake  Bay.  Chesapeake  Sci.  16:  62-66. 
Willis,  L.,  S.  T.  Threlkeld,  and  C.  C.  Carpenter.  1982.     Tail  loss  patterns 

in  Thamnoplus  (Reptilia:Colubridae)  and  the  probable  fate  of  injured 

individuals.  Copeia  1982:  98-101. 
Wilson,  K.  A.,  K.  L.  Heck,  Jr.,  and  K.  W.  Able.  1987.     Juvenile  blue 

crab.  Callinectes  siipuln.s.  survival:  an  evaluation  of  eelgrass,  Zostera 

manna,  as  refuge.  Fish.  Bull  85:  53-58. 
Wood,  F.  D.,  and  II.  F.  Wood,  II.  1932.     Autotomy  in  decapod  Crustacea. 

/  Exp.  Zool.  62:  1-55. 


Reference:  Bio/  Bull  180:  432-439.  (June,  1991) 


Hydration  State,  Metabolism,  and  Hatching 
of  Mono  Lake  Artemia  Cysts 

LAURIE  E.  DRINKWATER*  AND  JOHN  H.  CROWE 

Department  of  Zoology,  University  of  California.  Davis,  California 


Abstract.  Anemia  nionica.  the  only  macrozooplankton 
in  Mono  Lake,  California,  is  unique  among  brine  shrimp 
in  that  it  produces  encysted  diapause  embryos  that  sink 
to  the  lake  bottom  where  they  overwinter.  Currently,  the 
lake's  salinity  is  about  twice  as  high  as  it  was  40  years  ago 
and.  at  equilibrium,  it  is  projected  to  fluctuate  between 
169-248  g/1.  Here  we  describe  the  effects  of  salinity  on 
the  termination  of  diapause,  hatching,  carbohydrate  me- 
tabolism, and  hydration  of  the  cysts.  As  expected,  hatching 
is  much  more  sensitive  to  salinity  than  is  termination  of 
diapause.  Carbohydrate  metabolism,  which  involves  the 
conversion  of  trehalose  to  glycerol  and  is  required  for 
hatching,  responds  to  increasing  salinity  as  reported  in 
other  Artemia  species:  increasing  amounts  of  glycerol  must 
be  synthesized  as  salinity  is  raised.  The  unfreezable  water 
in  these  embryos  is  0.29  g  H:O/gram  dry  weight  (gdw) 
cysts,  similar  to  values  reported  for  other  biological  sys- 
tems. This  result  and  previous  studies  suggest  that  water 
probably  becomes  limiting  at  hydration  levels  of  about 
0.60  g  H:O/gdw  cysts.  In  Mono  Lake  water,  the  cysts 
reach  this  critical  hydration  at  a  salinity  between  140- 
160  g/1,  equivalent  to  approximately  3780-4330  mOsm/ 
kg.  We  conclude  that  Artemia  monica  will  cease  to  exist 
within  this  salinity  range  and  doubt  that  it  can  hatch  be- 
yond this  limit,  which  is  imposed  by  the  requirement  of 
metabolic  processes  for  minimal  amounts  of  cellular 
water. 

Introduction 

The  relationships  between  external  salinity,  metabolic 
activity,  and  the  physical  state  of  cellular  water  have  been 
studied  extensively  in  the  encysted  diapause  embryo  of 


Received  26  March  1990;  accepted  27  December  1990. 
*  Current  address:  Department  of  Vegetable  Crops,  University  of 
California.  Davis,  CA  95616. 


A.  franciscana  (Clegg.  1964,  1978,  1986;  Glasheen  and 
Hand,  1989).  In  their  natural  environment,  the  cysts  are 
frequently  subjected  to  extreme  fluctuations  in  salinity 
and  complete  desiccation.  Rupture  of  the  cyst  wall  during 
hatching  is  thought  to  be  an  osmotic  process,  brought 
about  by  the  synthesis  of  glycerol  and  the  resulting  increase 
in  turgor  pressure  (Clegg,  1964,  1976a).  Although  the  cysts 
can  hatch  in  a  wide  range  of  salinities  by  increasing  the 
amount  of  glycerol  produced  as  salinity  increases,  they 
reach  a  point  where  hatching  is  completely  inhibited  due 
to  inadequate  cellular  water  (Clegg,  1964). 

The  critical  hydration  levels  that  limit  metabolism  in 
A.  franciscana  have  been  investigated  by  Clegg  and  col- 
leagues (1974,  1976a,  b,  c;  1977;  Clegg  and  Cavagnaro, 
1976;  Clegg  and  Lovallo,  1977).  In  an  elegant  series  of 
studies,  they  report  that  the  shutdown  of  metabolism  due 
to  water  loss  occurs  in  a  step-wise  fashion,  with  distinct 
metabolic  transitions  corresponding  to  changes  in  the 
physical  state  of  water  remaining  in  the  cysts.  The  met- 
abolic characteristics  and  hydration  levels  of  these  three 
metabolic  domains  are  shown  in  Table  I.  Cysts  with  water 
contents  lower  than  about  0.60  HiO  g/gdw  (gram  dry 
weight)  exhibit  a  dramatic  decrease  in  their  metabolic  ca- 
pabilities. Further  studies  suggest  that  a  significant  bulk 
aqueous  phase  is  not  present  until  the  cysts  contain  more 
than  0.6  g  H:O/gdw.  Clegg  has  hypothesized  that,  as  hy- 
dration levels  fall  below  0.60  g  H:O/gdw,  metabolic  path- 
ways are  disconnected,  resulting  in  a  restricted  metabolism 
that  does  not  permit  hatching  of  the  cyst  (for  reviews  of 
the  model  see  Clegg,  1978,  1986).  Another  metabolic 
transition  occurs  at  hydration  levels  of  0.3  g  H2O/gdw 
and  lower.  This  water  is  considered  to  be  the  "bound 
water,"  and  at  water  contents  of  0.3  g/gdw  and  lower,  the 
only  metabolic  activity  evident  is  a  slow  decline  in  ATP. 

An  atypical  species  of  brine  shrimp,  A.  monica.  inhabits 
Mono  Lake,  California — a  large,  deep,  termial  lake  on 
the  eastern  side  of  the  Sierra  Nevada.  Unlike  A.  francis- 


432 


ARTEMIA  CYSTS,  HYDRATION  STATE 


433 


Table  1 
Hydration-dependence  nf  cellular  metabolism  in  Artemia  cysts 

Cyst  hydration 
(g  H:O/gdw 
cysts) 


Metabolic  events  initiated 


0  to  0. I 


None  observed 

Decrease  in  ATP  concentration 


0.1  to  0.3  +  0.05       No  additional  events  observed 

0.3  ±  0.05  Metabolism  involving  several  amino  acids,  K.rebs- 

cycle  and  related  intermediated,  short  chain 
aliphatic  acids,  pyrimidine  nucleotides,  slight 
decrease  in  glycogen  concentration 

0.3  to  0.6  ±  0.07       No  additional  events  observed 

0.6  ±  0.07  Cellular  respiration,  carbohydrate  synthesis, 

mobilization  of  trehalose,  net  increase  in  ATP. 
major  changes  in  the  free  amino  acid  pool, 
hydrolysis  of  yolk  protein,  RNA  and  protein 
synthesis,  resumption  of  embryonic 
development 

0.6  to  1 A  No  additional  events  observed 

The  two  critical  levels  of  hydration  where  large  changes  in  metabolic 
capacity  occur  are  shown  in  bold  typeface.  After  Clegg  (1978). 

cana  cysts.  Mono  Lake  cysts  are  not  subjected  to  desic- 
cation, and  rarely  experience  drastic  changes  in  salinity. 
The  thin  cyst  shell  permits  the  cysts  to  sink  to  the  lake 
bottom  where  they  are  activated  (diapause  is  terminated) 
by  the  cold  temperatures.  They  remain  on  the  lake  bottom 
for  6-7  months,  until  late  February  to  early  March,  when 
they  begin  hatching  (Lenz,  1980,  1983). 

The  future  viability  of  this  species  is  of  concern,  because 
water  exports  from  the  Mono  Lake  basin  have  caused  a 
decline  in  the  lake  level.  The  lake's  salinity  has  more  than 
doubled  over  the  past  40  years  and,  at  equilibrium,  it  is 
predicted  to  fluctuate  between  169  and  248  g/1  (Vorster, 
1985).  The  shrimp  are  the  only  macrozooplankton  in  the 
lake,  and  serve  as  a  food  source  for  the  large  population 
of  California  gulls  that  have  a  major  rookery  on  the  lake. 
Thus,  A.  monica  occupies  a  key  position  in  the  Mono 
Lake  ecosystem  (Mason  1967;  Winkler.  1977;  Lenz, 
1980). 

This  study  examines  the  effects  of  salinity  of  the  me- 
tabolism and  hydration  of  the  cysts  in  Anemia  monica. 
The  results  indicate  that  in  terms  of  their  response  to  el- 
evated salinity,  A.  monica  is  very  similar  to  the  well-stud- 
ied brine  shrimp,  A.  jranciscana,  despite  pronounced  dif- 
ferences in  the  habitats  of  these  two  species. 

Materials  and  Methods 

Collection  of  cysts 

Gravid  females  collected  from  Mono  Lake  were  held 
in  the  laboratory  for  1-2  weeks  while  they  released  their 


cysts.  Conditions  approximated  those  of  Mono  Lake  in 
the  summer:  temperature  was  18°C,  and  Mono  Lake  wa- 
ter (MLW)  containing  50  g  solids/1  ( 1 300  mOsm/kg)  was 
used;  no  food  was  provided.  Before  being  used,  the  cysts 
were  stored  anaerobically  in  this  medium  at  14°C.  The 
newly  released  diapause  cysts  will  not  hatch,  even  when 
placed  in  conditions  normally  favorable  for  hatching;  they 
require  a  cold  treatment  (<5°C)  of  about  90  days  (Dana, 
1981;  Drinkwater  and  Crowe.  1987).  Storage  at  14°C,  as 
described  above,  had  previously  maintained  the  diapause 
state  and  yielded  viable  cysts  (Drinkwater  and  Crowe, 
1987). 

Osmolality  of  Mono  Lake  water 

The  physiologically  relevant  measurement  of  the  dis- 
solved solids  in  this  water  is  osmolality,  as  we  will  show 
in  the  Results  section.  However,  previous  workers  have 
represented  their  results  in  terms  of  grams  of  dissolved 
solids  per  liter  of  water  (g/1).  To  facilitate  a  comparison 
of  the  present  results  with  those  of  previous  studies,  we 
will  report  data  here  in  both  forms.  Furthermore,  previous 
workers  have  referred  to  the  measurement  g/1  as  "salinity" 
even  though  this  is  not  the  precise  oceanographic  meaning 
of  this  word.  We  will  continue  this  usage. 

It  is  not  possible  to  reconstitute  Mono  Lake  water  from 
the  salts  collected  by  complete  evaporation  because  some 
insoluble  salts  are  formed  during  precipitation.  Conse- 
quently, we  produced  the  desired  salinities  by  partially 
evaporating  water  collected  from  the  lake  and  determining 
the  salt  content  of  subsamples  gravimetrically.  Adjust- 
ments were  made  to  the  non-desiccated  stock  by  dilution 
with  distilled  water  to  yield  MLWs  of  varying  salinity. 
The  osmolality  of  these  solutions  was  determined  by 
measurements  of  freezing  point  depression.  Samples  of 
the  solutions  were  frozen  in  an  ethylene  glycol  bath  chilled 
to  about  — 20°C.  The  temperature  was  monitored,  and 
the  equilibrium  freezing  point  was  recorded  during  the 
release  of  the  latent  heat  of  fusion  as  the  samples  froze. 
These  measurements  were  repeated  four  times  on  each 
sample. 

Metabolic  studies 

Preliminary  analyses  indicated  that  the  carbohydrate 
profile  of  A.  monica  cysts  was  essentially  identical  to  those 
of  other  Artemia  cysts:  prior  to  development  the  embryos 
contained  high  levels  of  trehalose  and  glycogen,  and  low 
levels  of  glycerol.  Therefore,  we  expected  that  trehalose 
would  be  mobilized  for  glycerol  production  during  pre- 
emergence  development  (FED). 

In  the  first  experiment,  we  determined  when  trehalose 
degradation  is  initiated.  Diapause  cysts  were  incubated 
aerobically  and  anaerobically  at  two  temperatures,  14°C 
and  4°C.  We  knew  from  our  previous  work  that  the  cysts 


434 


L.  E.  DRINKWATER  AND  J.  H.  CROWE 


in  the  14°C  incubations  would  not  break  diapause  and, 
consequently,  would  not  hatch.  However,  the  4°C  treat- 
ment would  permit  the  cysts  to  break  diapause,  and  they 
would  begin  hatching  if  adequate  oxygen  were  present 
(Drinkwater  and  Crowe,  1987).  The  inclusion  of  the  an- 
aerobic treatments  allowed  us  to  break  diapause  but  in- 
hibit hatching,  as  aerobic  metabolism  is  obligatory  for 
hatching  to  occur  (Clegg  and  Conte,  1980).  Because  the 
cysts  could  break  diapause  and  hatch  in  the  aerobic  4°C 
incubation,  we  set  up  parallel  groups  such  that  the  percent 
hatch  under  these  conditions  could  be  monitored  (Drink- 
water  and  Crowe,  1987).  This  first  experiment  enabled  us 
to  compare  the  carbohydrate  metabolism  of  cysts  re- 
maining in  diapause  (14°C)  to  that  of  cysts  which  had 
terminated  diapause  (4°C)  and  resumed  development. 

The  effects  of  salinity  on  carbohydrate  metabolism  were 
studied  as  follows.  Diapause  cysts  were  incubated  aero- 
bically  at  4°C  in  MLW  of  four  salinities:  50,  80,  100,  and 
125  g/1  (1300,  2100,  2690,  and  3370  mOsm/kg).  Two 
petri  dishes  of  cysts  were  set  up  at  each  salinity  and  main- 
tained in  hygrostats  over  water  of  the  same  salinity.  The 
media  were  monitored  with  a  refractometer  to  assure 
constant  salinity.  Under  these  conditions,  cysts  can  break 
diapause  and  begin  hatching,  thus  percent  hatch  was 
monitored  as  described  above.  In  addition,  to  separate 
the  effects  of  salinity  on  termination  of  diapause  from 
those  on  hatching,  subsamples  were  periodically  taken 
and  placed  under  favorable  hatching  conditions:  14°C, 
in  MLW  of  50  g/1  ( 1 300  mOsm/kg). 

Carbohydrate  assays 

Samples  of  cysts  were  removed  and  decapsulated  by 
exposure  to  2%  hypochlorite  (diluted  household  bleach), 
at  4°C,  until  examination  under  a  dissecting  microscope 
showed  that  the  cyst  shell  had  been  removed  (Sorgeloos 
et  al,,  1977).  This  usually  required  5  to  10  min.  Each 
sample  was  divided  into  three  subsamples  and  weighed 
after  desiccation  over  CaSO4.  Trehalose,  glycerol,  and 
glucose  were  extracted  by  grinding  cysts  in  a  tissue  ho- 
mogenizer  in  60%  ethanol.  Soluble  sugars  were  separated 
by  high  pressure  liquid  chromatography  (HPLC)  on  a 
HPX-87H  anion  exchange  column  (Bio-Rad;  Schwarz- 
enbach,  1982)  and  were  quantified  with  a  Knauer  differ- 
ential refractometer.  The  pellet  was  analyzed  forglycogen 
according  to  the  anthrone  method  (Umbreit  et  al.,  1972). 

Calorimetry:  determination  of  unfreeiable  water 

A  Perkin-Elmer  DSC2-C  Differential  Scanning  Calo- 
rimeter, supplemented  with  a  Perkin-Elmer  3600  data 
station  and  TADS  thermal  analysis  software,  was  used  to 
determine  the  amount  of  unfreezable  water  in  hydrated 
cysts  of  A.  monicaandA.franciscana.  Decapsulated  cysts 
were  hydrated  to  varying  degrees,  either  by  submersion 


in  distilled  water,  or  by  exposure  to  water  vapor  in  indi- 
vidual hygrostats  (Clegg,  1974).  Cysts  hydrated  in  the  liq- 
uid phase  required  thorough  blotting  to  remove  all  water 
on  their  surfaces.  We  sometimes  observed  two  endo- 
thermic  spikes  due  to  water  on  the  surface  of  the  cysts: 
these  samples  were  discarded. 

The  majority  of  the  data  presented  in  this  paper  are 
from  cysts  hydrated  from  the  vapor  phase.  Samples  (8 
mg)  were  placed  in  pre-weighed  aluminum  calorimetry 
pans  and  sealed.  The  pans  were  weighed  and  the  amount 
of  freezable  water  was  then  measured  by  freezing  the  cysts, 
allowing  them  to  reach  thermal  equilibrium  at  -63°C, 
and  running  calorimetry  scans  from  -63°C  to  27°C. 

Frozen  water  in  the  cysts  was  quantified  by  comparing 
the  enthalpy  of  the  melting  endotherm  for  water  in  the 
frozen  cysts  (calculated  by  the  Perkin-Elmer  TADS  soft- 
ware), with  enthalpy  of  known  standards  treated  in  the 
same  way.  After  calorimetry,  the  pans  were  punctured  to 
permit  desiccation  of  the  cysts  by  lyophilization.  The 
samples  were  reweighed  after  equilibrating  over  CaSO4. 
The  water  content  of  the  samples  was  determined  as  the 
difference  between  the  wet  and  dry  weights. 

Hydration  of  cysts  in  Mono  Lake  water 

The  water  content  of  cysts  as  a  function  of  the  salinities 
of  MLW  was  determined  according  to  the  method  of  Clegg 
(1974).  Cysts  were  placed  in  individual  chambers  con- 
structed of  35  ml  covered  vials  and  hydrated  from  the 
vapor  phase  over  MLW  of  50,  80,  100,  125,  140,  160, 
and  200  g/1.  Six  days  were  needed  for  cysts  in  the  vapor 
phase  to  reach  equilibrium  at  2°C  (Clegg,  1974).  After 
being  weighed,  the  cysts  were  lyophilized  and  then  brought 
to  equilibrium  over  CaSO4  for  10  h  in  individual  desic- 
cators; the  dry  weight  was  then  determined. 

Results  and  Discussion 

Salinity  of  Mono  Lake  water:  a  clarification 

The  salinities  of  MLW  and  its  dilutions  have,  in  the 
past,  been  compared  with  those  of  solutions  of  entirely 
different  ionic  compositions  such  as  seawater  or  NaCl  so- 
lutions (Dana,  1981;  Dana  and  Lenz,  1986).  Mono  Lake 
water  has  an  unusual  salt  composition,  with  high  levels 
of  carbonates  and  sulfates  (Cole  and  Brown,  1967).  Be- 
cause the  cysts  respond  to  the  chemical  potential  of  water, 
we  clearly  cannot  make  direct  comparisons  of  solutions 
containing  different  ionic  species  on  a  g/1  basis.  Such 
comparisons  have  led  to  considerable  confusion  in  the 
literature.  Our  careful  measurements  of  the  osmolality  of 
diluted  Mono  Lake  water  and  NaCl  solutions  illustrate 
this  point  (Fig.  1 ).  The  osmolality  of  MLW  is  lower  than 
a  solution  of  NaCl  containing  the  same  amount  of  salts 
by  weight.  Thus,  the  water  content  of  cysts  in  Mono  Lake 


ARTEMIA  CYSTS,  HYDRATION  STATE 


435 


water  is  higher  than  those  in  the  NaCl  solution  containing 
the  same  amount  of  salts  on  a  g/1  basis.  This  seemingly 
simple  point  is  exceedingly  important,  in  that  interpre- 
tation of  data  based  on  dissolved  solutes  can  lead  to  in- 
correct conclusions,  as  illustrated  by  the  following  dis- 
cussion. 

Previous  workers  have  shown  that  A.  monica  can  hatch 
in  MLW  of  133  g/1  (Dana  and  Lenz,  1986).  Limits  for 
many  Anemia  populations  are  between  70-99  g  NaCl/1, 
and  because  the  highest  salinity  for  hatching  (A.  francis- 
cana,  Utah  population)  is  reported  as  >99  g  NaCl/1 
(d'Agostino,  1965,  as  cited  by  Collins,  1977).  Dana  and 
Lenz  concluded  that  A.  monica  is  unusual  in  its  ability 
to  hatch  at  increased  salinities.  However,  Figure  1  shows 
that  133  g/1  MLW  is  equivalent  to  a  NaCl  solution  of 
about  105  g/1  (3500  mOsm) — very  similar  to  the  highest 
reported  salinity  permitting  hatching  in  A.  franciscana. 
We  conclude  that  A.  monica  is  not  unusual  with  regard 
to  its  hatchability  as  a  function  of  osmolality  of  the  bathing 
solution. 

Salinity  effects  on  the  hatching  mechanism 
in  A.  monica 

As  shown  in  Figure  2,  breakdown  of  trehalose  into 
glycerol  only  occurs  under  aerobic  conditions  in  cysts  that 
are  able  to  break  diapause  and  hatch.  Therefore,  the  same 
osmotic  mechanism  proposed  for  hatching  in  A.  francis- 
cana is  likely  present  in  A.  monica. 


250- 


5000 


n 
O 

E 


0  50  100  150  200 

Grams/liter 

Figure  1.  Osmolality  of  NaCl  solutions  and  Mono  Lake  water  of 
equal  salt  content  on  a  g/1  basis.  Dotted  arrows  indicate  that  a  2.0  A/ 
(117  g/1)  NaCl  solution  is  the  osmotic  equivalent  of  about  150  g/1  Mono 
Lake  water.  NaCl  data  are  from  Weast  (1983).  Data  for  Mono  Lake 
water  are  from  two  sources  using  different  methods:  (squares)  our  data, 
obtained  by  freezing  point  depression;  (circles)  Herbst  and  Dana  (1980), 
determined  by  vapor  pressure  osmometry. 


« 
To 
o 


O) 

E 
6) 


ft  of  days 

Figure  2.  Trehalose  (solid  lines)  and  glycerol  (broken  lines)  contents 
of  cysts  incubated  under  the  following  conditions:  4°C.  aerobic  (triangles): 
4°C,  anaerobic  (closed  circles);  14°C,  aerobic  (open  circles);  14°C.  an- 
aerobic (squares).  The  4°C.  aerobic  incubation  is  the  only  treatment 
which  permitted  both  activation  and  hatching;  the  cysts  were  not  sampled 
after  90  days  because  the  majority  of  them  had  hatched.  Note  the  sig- 
nificant decline  in  trehalose  as  glycerol  increases.  The  other  three  incu- 
bations exhibited  no  hatching:  cysts  in  the  4°C,  anaerobic  incubation 
could  break  diapause,  but  could  not  hatch  in  the  absence  of  oxygen. 
Points  are  x  ±  SD.  n  =  3. 


Having  shown  that  the  synthesis  of  glycerol  proceeds 
with  hatching,  we  incubated  cysts  in  several  salinities  of 
MLW  to  determine  the  effect  of  salinity  on  carbohydrate 
metabolism.  Simultaneously,  we  monitored  termination 
of  diapause  and  hatching.  Increasing  salinities  resulted  in 
faster  synthesis  of  glycerol  (Fig.  3a),  while  trehalose 
breakdown  is  slower  at  higher  salinities  (Fig.  3b).  Figure 
3c  shows  that,  at  the  lowest  salinity  (50  g/1),  glycogen  is 
synthesized  in  addition  to  glycerol,  suggesting  that  at  this 
salinity  some  of  the  trehalose  is  being  converted  to  gly- 
cogen. However,  glycogen  shows  net  degradation  in  the 
higher  salinities,  indicating  that  an  osmotic  regulatory 
mechanism  may  control  the  amount  of  glycerol  and  gly- 
cogen synthesized  by  the  embryo  in  response  to  changes 
in  salinity.  The  decline  of  glycogen  may,  in  part,  also  ex- 
plain the  lower  hatch  seen  in  Figure  4  at  higher  salinities. 
If  the  embryo  must  synthesize  more  glycerol  to  hatch  as 
salinity  increases,  fewer  carbohydrate  reserves  will  be 
available  for  other  necessary  developmental  processes. 

Salinity  effects  on  termination  of  diapause 

Figure  5  indicates  that  there  is  only  a  slight  inhibition 
of  diapause  termination  in  the  highest  salinity  used  in 
these  experiments.  The  percent  hatch  is  essentially  the 
same  in  three  of  the  salinities,  with  only  a  20%  lower 
hatch  in  the  cysts  from  the  125  g/1  (3370  mOsm/kg)  treat- 
ment. When  data  from  the  metabolic  studies  are  com- 
bined with  the  hatching  data  from  this  study  and  others 
(Dana  and  Lenz,  1986),  we  can  conclude  that  the  decrease 


436 


L.  E.  DRINKWATER  AND  J.  H.  CROWE 


in 

> 
_ 


•o 
E 


0) 

o 


80- 


a> 

s- 


U> 

~ 


ra 

<1) 


>. 
O 


a. 

O) 

o 

£ 

O) 

at 


#  of  days 

Figure  3.  Changes  in  carbohydrate  levels  of  cysts  incubated  aero- 
bically  in  four  salinities  of  Mono  Lake  water  at  4°C.  (A)  Glycerol  synthesis 
is  faster  at  higher  salinities.  (B)  Trehalose  breakdown  is  faster  in  the 
lower  salinities.  (C)  Glycogen  levels;  only  the  lowest  salinity,  50  g/l  ( 1 300 
mOsm/kg).  shows  a  net  increase  in  glycogen.  Points  are  x  ±  SD,  n  =  3. 
Refer  to  Figure  4  to  determine  percent  hatch  during  the  experiment. 


in  hatching  observed  at  the  higher  salinities  used  in  these 
experiments  primarily  results  from  interference  with 
hatching  rather  than  release  from  diapause.  Thus,  ter- 
mination of  diapause  is  less  susceptible  to  increasing  sa- 


o 

ro 


C 

<u 
o 

0) 
Q. 


100  150 


#  of   days 

Figure  4.  Percent  hatch  of  Anemia  monica  during  the  experiment 
described  in  Figure  3.  Hatching  in  the  50  g/l  ( 1 300  mOsm/kg)  treatment 
was  essentially  the  same  as  the  80  g/l  (2 100  mOsm/kg)  treatment  and  is 
not  shown.  Each  point  represents  x  ±  SD,  n  =  3. 


Unities,  because  salinities  that  only  impair  cyst  activation 
completely  inhibit  hatching  (Dana  and  Lenz,  1986; 
Drinkwater  and  Crowe,  1987). 

Unfreezable  water  and  hydration  of  Mono  Lake  cysts 

Previous  experiments  indicate  that  the  hatching  limit 
of  A.  monica  is  somewhere  between  133-159  g/l  MLW 
(Dana  and  Lenz,  1986).  Based  on  our  metabolic  data  and 
studies  of  the  hydration  dependence  of  metabolism,  we 
assume  that,  at  this  limiting  salinity,  conventional  me- 
tabolism can  no  longer  occur,  and  degradation  of  trehalose 
therefore  stops.  However,  the  potential  for  adaptations 
permitting  the  cysts  to  hatch  at  higher  salinities  is  a  pos- 
sibility to  which  several  researchers  working  at  Mono  Lake 
have  alluded  (Dana,  pers.  comm.).  We  have  attempted  to 


ra 

.c 

c 

(V 

o 

h_ 
0) 
Q. 


52 


#  of  days 


Figure  5.  Percent  hatch  of  cysts  removed  from  the  treatment  salinity 
and  hatched  in  50  g/l  (1300  mOsm/kg)at  14°C  to  determine  the  number 
of  activated  cysts,  x  ±  SD  of  three  determinations  are  graphed. 


ARTEMIA  CYSTS,  HYDRATION  STATE 


437 


1  6- 


« 

to 

o        10- 
3 

O)          08- 

& 

5          06- 


TO 


04- 


y  =,  0  28901  »  1  0887x    R"2  =  0  995  0^ 


02       04        06 


10        12 


g  ice/gdw  cysts 

Figure  6.  Unfreezable  water  in  Anemia  monica  (open  circles)  and 
A.  franciscana  (closed  circles).  The  line  represents  a  linear  regression  on 
the  A.  monica  data:  the  .-I-  franciscana  data  are  included  for  comparison. 
The  y-intercept  (unfreezable  water)  is  0.29  g  H:O/gdw  cysts. 


determine  the  physical  limitation  of  conventional  metab- 
olism in  these  organisms  by  studying  their  hydration 
properties  and  unfreezable  water  content. 

The  amount  of  unfreezable  water  in  A.  monica  cysts 
corresponds  closely  with  our  data  for  A.  franciscana  (Fig. 
6).  The  y-intercept  gives  the  estimated  amount  of  un- 
freezable water,  in  this  case,  0.29  g  H:O/gdw  for  A.  monica 
cysts.  A  few  A.  franciscana  samples  were  run  for  com- 
parison. Linear  regression  of  these  points  estimates  un- 
freezable water  to  be  0.28  g/gdw  cysts,  very  close  to  our 
value  for  A.  monica.  These  values  for  unfreezable  water 
coincide  closely  with  the  critical  hydration  at  which  the 
transition  to  the  ametabolic  state  occurs,  about  0.3  g/gdw 
(Table  I;  Clegg,  1978,  review).  Below  this  water  content, 
the  cysts  are  considered  to  be  ametabolic.  Thus,  the  un- 
freezable water  content  represents  a  physiologically  sig- 
nificant hydration  feature  of  the  cells. 

Comparing  the  quantity  of  bound  water  contained  in 
these  two  species  (0.28-0.29  g/gdw)  with  figures  reported 
for  a  wide  range  of  biological  systems,  we  find  close  agree- 
ment; amounts  range  from  0.3  to  0.5  g/gdw  (Williams, 
1970;  Cooke  and  Kuntz,  1974,  Garlid,  1978;  O'Dell  and 
Crowe,  1 979;  Crowe  elai,  1983).  Our  results  do  not  agree 
with  previous  findings  for  a  now  extinct  population  of  A. 
franciscana  previously  located  in  Brazil;  freezable  water 
in  that  study  was  about  0.6  g/gdw  (Crowe  el  ai,  1981). 
However,  because  the  phase  transition  curve  for  the  A. 
franciscana  cysts  was  reported  to  be  curvilinear  rather  than 
linear,  the  cysts  probably  had  water  in  their  outer  porous 
shells,  causing  the  internal  water  content  to  appear  higher 


Table  II 

H 'ater  content  (g  H^O/grains  dry  weight)  qfdecapsulated  Anemia 
monica  cyslx  in  Mono  Lake  water  of  salinities  ranging 
from  50  to  200  g/l 


Salinity 

(g/D 

Cyst  hydration 
g  HiO/gdw  cysts 

50 

1.  16  ±0.06 

80 

1.04  ±0.08 

100 

0.97  ±0.15 

125 

0.76  ±0.01 

140 

0.66  ±  0.02 

160 

0.55  ±  0.04 

200 

0.39  ±  0.02 

Data  reported  as  \  ±  S.D.,  n  =  3. 


than  it  actually  was.  In  the  previous  experiments  with 
Anemia  (Crowe  el  al.  1981),  the  cysts  were  hydrated  by 
immersion  in  water,  whereas  in  our  present  experiments, 
hydration  was  achieved  by  exposure  to  the  vapor  phase, 
eliminating  this  potential  source  of  error. 

Salinity  and  water  content  of  Mono  Lake  cysts 

Finally,  the  water  contents  of  A.  monica  cysts  in  varying 
concentrations  of  Mono  Lake  water  were  determined  to 
assess  the  point  at  which  hatching  would  be  limited  by 
insufficient  intracellular  water.  In  Table  II,  the  hydration 
levels  of  A.  monica  cysts  equilibrated  in  MLW  of  50-200 
g/l  are  reported.  To  permit  comparison  with  A.  francis- 
cana, previously  published  data  have  been  corrected  for 
the  presence  of  the  shell  and  have  been  graphed  with  .-1. 
monica  in  Figure  7;  a  close  correspondence  in  water  con- 
tents is  demonstrated.  A.  monica  cysts  reach  a  critical 
hydration  of  0.6-0.67  g/gdw  in  salinities  between  140- 


1.4  - 

• 

TJ 
D) 

1.2- 

1         'I       1 

O 

1.0  - 

•f 

1 

08  - 

1 

•                           0 

3 

*                         °     • 

c 

06  - 

5 

o 

v~ 

0.4  - 

<Q 

O      ^  numuro 

•0 

x" 

0.2  - 

•     .'^  fratuucana 

0.0  T  '  1  '  1  '  1           '           i          • 
0                1000            2000            3000            4000            50 

mOsm/kg 

Figure  7.  Hydration  of  Anemia  monica  cysts  compared  to  A.  fran- 
ciscana cysts  as  a  function  of  osmolality.  Data  from  A .  franciscana  have 
been  corrected  for  shell  weight  (from  Clegg,  1974,  1976a). 


438 


L.  E.  DRINKWATER  AND  J.  H.  CROWE 


160  g/1  MLW,  equivalent  to  NaCl  solutions  of  approxi- 
mately 1.9-2.1  M  (3600-4050  mOsm/kg). 

Clegg  ( 1978)  has  not  detected  conventional  metabolism 
in  A.  franciscana  cysts  hydrated  in  2.0  molal  NaCl.  More 
recently,  Glasheen  and  Hand  (1989)  have  used  micro- 
calorimetry  to  demonstrate  that  heat  dissipation,  and  thus 
metabolism,  in  A.  franciscana  from  the  Great  Salt  Lake 
is  severely  depressed  by  2.0  M  NaCl.  We  suggest  that  A. 
monica  also  experiences  a  critical  hydration  at  this  salinity, 
and  submit  that  conventional  metabolism  will  not  occur 
in  these  cysts  at  a  limiting  salinity  between  140-160  g/1 
MLW.  We  must  stress,  however,  that  at  salinities  some- 
what lower  that  these,  which  actually  impose  a  physical 
limit  to  conventional  metabolism,  hatching  will  be  im- 
paired; i.e.,  a  smaller  proportion  of  cysts  will  hatch,  and 
hatching  will  take  longer  (Jennings  and  Whitaker,  1941; 
Clegg,  1964;  Dana  and  Lenz,  1986). 

Conclusions 

Several  lines  of  evidence  presented  here  suggest  that 
cysts  of  A.  monica  possess  limits  to  development  that  are 
similar  to  those  found  in  A.  franciscana.  (1)  Hatching  is 
correlated  with  the  synthesis  of  glycerol  and,  as  in  A.  fran- 
ciscana, synthesis  of  this  compound  is  probably  required 
for  hatching.  Glycerol  synthesis  increases  when  the  cysts 
are  incubated  at  higher  salinities.  (2)  The  amounts  of  un- 
freezable  water  in  A.  monica  and  A.  franciscana  are  sim- 
ilar, suggesting  that  the  hydration  levels  at  which  meta- 
bolic transitions  occur  are  the  same.  (3)  At  salinities  of 
about  150  g/1  MLW  (equivalent  to  4060  mOsm/kg),  the 
water  content  of  A.  monica  cysts  is  less  than  0.6  g/gdw, 
thus  conventional  metabolism  and  development  will  not 
be  possible. 

We  conclude  that,  while  .-1.  monica  can  hatch  in  salin- 
ities in  the  upper  range  of  those  reported  for  Anemia  cysts, 
they  are  not  unique  in  this  ability,  and  they  have  no  un- 
usual adaptive  potential  with  respect  to  salinity  thresholds. 
All  available  evidence  suggests  that  these  limits  on  me- 
tabolism are  imposed  by  the  biophysical  interactions  in- 
herent in  the  hydration  of  cellular  components  and  the 
effects  of  this  water  of  hydration  on  the  functioning  of 
macromolecular  assemblages  (Clegg,  1986;  Glasheen  and 
Hand,  1989). 

Thus,  these  organisms  probably  cannot  adapt  to  Mono 
Lake  salinities  above  about  150  g/1  (4060  mOsm/kg)  by 
extending  their  hatching  limit  beyond  that  level.  Biological 
adaptation,  powerful  as  it  is,  cannot  overcome  the  basic 
principles  of  eukaryotic  metabolism  which  require  the 
presence  of  minimal  amounts  of  cellular  water.  It  follows 
then,  that  A.  monica  will  become  extinct  when  salinity 
rises  to  between  1 40- 1 60  g/1 — even  before  the  lake  reaches 
equilibrium.  Certainly,  A.  monica  will  not  exist  in  Mono 
Lake  when  it  reaches  its  projected  equilibrium,  since  sa- 


linity will  then  be  169-248  g/1,  well  above  4000  mOsm/ 
kg  (Fig.  1). 

Finally,  should  the  salinities  in  Mono  Lake  be  allowed 
to  reach  these  levels,  we  doubt  that  another  brine  shrimp 
species  could  be  successfully  introduced  due  to  the  char- 
acteristics of  this  lake  that  make  it  unique  among  Anemia 
habitats  (Lenz,  1980;  Dana,  1981;  Bowen  et  ai,  1985; 
Drinkwater  and  Crowe,  1987).  First,  because  of  its  ionic 
composition,  Mono  Lake  water  is  toxic  to  many  Anemia 
populations,  including  the  most  well-known  North 
American  species,  A.  franciscana  (Bowen  et  al..  1985). 
Second,  in  order  for  a  species  to  persist  in  the  lake,  its 
life-cycle  would  need  to  be  synchronized  with  the  con- 
ditions in  Mono  Lake;  i.e..  diapause  induction  and  ter- 
mination must  occur  at  the  appropriate  times.  In  addition, 
the  Mono  Lake  ecosystem  probably  cannot  mimic  other 
hypersaline  lakes,  such  as  the  Great  Salt  Lake,  in  which 
floating  cysts  are  deposited  on  the  shore  as  the  lake  recedes 
and  are  then  swept  back  into  the  lake  by  spring  rains.  The 
average  annual  precipitation  in  the  Mono  basin  is  33  cm, 
compared  to  52  cm  for  the  Great  Salt  Lake.  And  in  the 
spring,  average  precipitation  for  Mono  Lake  is  only  4  cm 
(April,  May,  and  June),  while  the  Great  Salt  Lake  receives 
16  cm  during  these  same  months  (NOAA,  1985,  1986). 
These  observations  illustrate  some  of  the  specific  diffi- 
culties involved  in  attempting  to  introduce  a  replacement 
brine  shrimp  species  into  Mono  Lake. 

Acknowledgments 

Appreciation  is  extended  to  Gail  Dana  for  her  essential 
assistance  early  in  the  project.  We  are  sincerely  grateful 
to  Dr.  James  Clegg  for  his  contribution  of  many  enlight- 
ening discussions  and  for  reviewing  the  manuscript  prior 
to  submission.  This  research  was  supported  in  part  by  Los 
Angeles  Department  of  Water  and  Power. 

Literature  Cited 

Bowen,  S.  T.,  E.  A.  Fogarino,  K.  N.  Htchner,  G.  L.  Dana,  V.  H.  S.  Chow, 
M.  R.  Buoncristiani,  and  J.  R.  Carl.  1985.  Ecological  isolation  in 
Anemia,  population  differences  in  tolerance  of  anion  concentrations. 
J.  Cruxt.  Biol.  5:  126-129. 

Clegg,  J.  S.  1964.  The  control  of  emergence  and  metabolism  by  external 
osmotic  pressure  and  the  role  of  free  glycerol  in  developing  cysts  of 
Anemia  salina.  J  Exp.  Bmi  41:  879-892. 

Clegg,  J.  S.  1974.  Interrelationships  between  water  and  cellular  me- 
tabolism in  cysts.  I.  Hydration-dehydration  from  liquid  and  vapor 
phases.  J.  Exp.  Biol.  61:  291-308. 

Clegg,  J.  S.  1976a.  Interrelationships  between  water  and  metabolism 
in  Anemia  cysts.  II.  Carbohydrates.  Comp.  Biochem.  Physio/,  53a: 
83-87. 

Clegg,  J.  S.  1976b.  Interrelationships  between  water  and  cellular  me- 
tabolism in  Anemia  cysts.  III.  Respiration.  Comp.  Biochem.  Physiol 
53a:  89-92. 

Clegg,  J.  S.  1976c.  Interrelationships  between  water  and  cellular  me- 
tabolism in  Anemia  cysts.  V.  l4CO;-incorporation.  J.  Cell.  Physiol. 
89:  369-380. 


ARiKMlA  CYSTS.  HYDRAT1ON  STATE 


439 


Clegg,  J.  S.  1977.  Interrelationships  between  water  and  cellular  me- 
tabolism in  Anemia  cysts.  VI.  RNA  and  protein  synthesis.  J.  Cell. 
Physiol.  91:  143-154. 

Clegg,  J.  S.  1978.  Hydration-dependent  metabolism  transitions  and 
the  state  of  cellular  water  in  Anemia.  Pp.  1 17-154  in  Dry  Biological 
Systems.  J.  H.  Crowe  and  J.  S.  Clegg,  eds.  Academic  Press.  New 
York. 

Clegg,  J.  S.  1986.  The  physical  properties  and  metabolic  status  ofAr- 
tcmia  cysts  at  low  water  contents:  the  "water  replacement"  hypothesis. 
Pp.  169-185  in  Membranes,  Metabolism  and  Dry  Organisms,  A.  C. 
Leopold,  ed.  Cornell  University  Press.  Ithaca.  NY. 

Clegg,  J.  S.,  and  F.  P.  Conte.  1980.  A  review  of  the  cellular  and  de- 
velopmental biology  of  Anemia.  Pp.  11-54  in  The  Brine  Shrimp 
Anemia.  I'd/.  2.  Physiology,  Biochemistry.  Molecular  Biology.  G. 
Persoone,  P.  Sorgeloos,  O.  Roels,  and  E.  Jaspers,  eds.  Universa  Press, 
Wetteren.  Belgium. 

Clegg,  J.  S.,  and  J.  Cavagnaro.  1976.  Interrelationships  between  water 
and  cellular  metabolism  in  Anemia  cysts.  IV.  Adenosine  5'-triphos- 
phate  and  cysts  hydration.  J.  Cell.  Physiol.  88:  1 59- 1 66. 

Clegg,  J.  S.,  and  J.  Lovallo.  1977.  Interrelationships  between  water 
and  cellular  metabolism  in  Anemia  cysts.  VII.  Free  amino  acids.  J. 
Cell.  Physiol.  93:  161-168. 

Cole,  G.  A.,  and  R.  J.  Brown.  1967.  The  chemistry  of  Anemia  habitats. 
Ecology  4S:  858-861. 

Collins,  N.  C.  1977.  Ecological  studies  of  terminal  lakes:  their  relevance 
to  problems  in  limnology  and  population  biology.  Pp.  41  1-42(1  in 
Desertic  Terminal  Lakes.  Proceedings.  D.  C.  Greer.  ed.  Utah  Water 
Research  Laboratory,  Utah. 

Cooke,  R.,  and  I.  D.  kunlz.  1974.  The  properties  of  water  in  biological 
systems.  Anmi.  Rev.  Biophys.  Bioeng.  3:  95-126. 

Crowe,  J.  H.,  L.  M.  Crowe,  and  S.  J.  O'Dell.  1981.  Ice  formation 
during  freezing  of  Anemia  cysts  of  variable  water  contents.  Mol. 
Physiol.  1:  145-152. 

Crowe,  J.  H.,  S.  J.  Jackson,  and  L.  M.  Crowe.  1983.  Nonfreezable 
water  in  anhydrobiotic  nematodes.  Mol.  Physio/.  3:  99-105. 

D'Agostino,  A.  1965.  Comparative  studies  of  Anemia  salina  (devel- 
opment and  physiology).  Ph.D.  Diss.  New  York  University.  Cited 
by  Collins.  1977. 

Dana,  G.  1981.  Comparative  population  ecology  of  the  brine  shnmp. 
Anemia.  M.  A.  Thesis.  San  Francisco  State  University. 

Dana,  G.  L.,  and  P.  H.  Lenz.  1986.  Effects  of  increasing  salinity  on  an 
Anemia  population  from  Mono  Lake.  California.  Oecolgia  68:  428- 
436. 

Drinkwater,  L.  E.,  and  J.  H.  Crowe.  1987.  Regulation  of  embryonic 
diapause  in  Anemia:  environmental  and  physiological  signals.  /  Exp. 
Zoo/.  241:  297-307. 


Garlid,  K.  D.  1978.     Overview  of  our  understanding  of  intracellular 

water  in  hydrated  cells.  Pp.  3-19  in  Dry  Biological  Systems.  J.  H. 

Crowe  and  J.  S.  Clegg.  eds.  Academic  Press,  New  York. 
Glasheen,  J.  S.,  and  Hand,  S.  C.  1989.     Metabolic  heat  dissipation  and 

internal  solute  levels  of  Anemia  embryos  during  changes  in  cell- 
associated  water.  /  E.\r  Biol.  145:  263-282. 
Herbst,  D.  B.,  and  G.  L.  Dana.  1980.     Environmental  physiology  of 

salt  tolerance  in  an  alkaline  salt  lake  population  of  Anemia  from 

Mono  Lake,  California.  LI.S.A.  Pp.  157-167  in  The  Brine  Shrimp 

Anemia.  I  'ol.  ?.  Physiology.  Biochemistry,  .Molecular  Biology.  G. 

Persoone,  P.  Sorgeloos.  O.  Roels,  and  E.  Jaspers,  eds.  Universa  Press, 

Wetteren,  Belgium. 
Jennings,  R.  II.,  ami  I).  M.  Whitaker.  1941.     The  effect  of  salinity  upon 

the  rate  of  excystment  of  Anemia.  Biol.  Bull.  80:  194-201. 
I^enz,  P.  H.  1980.     Ecology  of  an  alkali-adapted  variety  of  Anemia  from 

Mono  Lake.  California.  LISA.  Pp.  79-96  in  The  Brine  Shrimp  Ar- 

lemia.  I'ol.  3.  Ecology,  Ciiln/nng.  Use  in  Auiiacu/liire.  G.  Persoone. 

P.  Sorgeloos.  O.  Roels.  and  E.  Jaspers,  eds.  Universa  Press,  Wetteren. 

Belgium. 
Mason,  D.  T.  1967.     Limnology  of  Mono  Lake,  California.  University 

of  California  Publications  in  Zoology'.  I  'ol.  S3.  University  of  California 

Press.  Berkeley. 
NOAA,  National  Climatic  Data  Center.  1985.     Climatology  of  the  US. 

No.  20:  Climatic  Summaries  of  Selected  Sites  1951-80:  Utah. 
NOAA,  National  Climatic  Data  Center.  1986.     Climatological  Data: 

California  Section;  Annual  Summary.  90:13. 
O'Dell,  S.  J.,  and  J.  H.  Crowe.  1979.     Freezing  in  nematodes:  the  effects 

of  variable  water  contents.  Cryobiology  16:  534-541. 
Schwarzenbach,  R.  1982.     High-performance  liquid  chromatography 

of  carboxylic  acids. ./.  Chromatogr.  251:  339-358. 
Sorgeloos,  P.,  E.  Bossuyt,  E.  Lavina,  M.  Baeza-Mesa,  and  G.  Peronne. 

1977.     Decapsulation  of  Anemia  cysts:  a  simple  technique  for  the 

improvement  of  the  use  of  brine  shrimp  in  aquaculture.  Aquaculture 

12:  311-315. 
Umbriet,   W.    M.,   R.    H.    Burris,   and   J.   F.   Stauffer,   eds.    1972. 

Manometric  and  Biochemical  Techn/i/ues.  5th  ed.  Burgess  Publishing 

Co..  Minneapolis.  MN. 
Vorster,  P.  T.  1985.     A  water  balance  forecast  model  for  Mono  Lake. 

California.  Masters  thesis.  California  State  University,  Hayward,  CA. 
Weast,  R.  C.,  ed.  1983.     CRC  Handbook  of  Chemistry  &  Physics.  CRC 

Press.  Boca  Raton.  FL. 
Williams,  R.  J.  1970.     Freezing  tolerance  in  Mytilus  edit/is.  Camp. 

Biochem.  Physiol.  35:  145-161. 

\Vinkler,  D.  W.,  ed.  1977.     An  ecological  study  of  Mono  Lake,  Cali- 
fornia. Institute  of  Ecology.  University  of  California,  Davis,  Publ. 

No.  12. 


Reference:  Biol.  Hull.  180:  440-446.  (June.  1991) 


Ultrastructure  and  Neuronal  Control  of  Luminous 
Cells  in  the  Copepod  Gaussia  princeps 

MARK  R.  BOWLBY1  AND  JAMES  F.  CASE 

Marine  Science  Institute  and  Department  of  Biological  Sciences, 
University  of  California.  Santa  Barbara.  California  93106 


Abstract.  The  physiology  of  light  production  in  cope- 
pods  is  largely  unknown.  The  mesopelagic  copepod 
Gaussia  princeps  possesses  luminous  glands,  each  con- 
sisting of  a  single  large  cell  discharging  through  a  cuticular 
pore.  Slow  flashes  external  to  the  cuticle  are  triggered  from 
excised  abdomens  by  electrical  stimulation  of  the  ventral 
nerve  cord.  Each  luminous  cell  contains  UV  fluorescent 
secretory  vesicles  distally,  which  are  secreted  through  a 
valved  cuticular  pore.  Each  luminous  cell,  except  for  the 
most  proximal  portion,  is  surrounded  by  a  cellular  sheath, 
which  appears  to  form  the  distal  valve.  Luminous  cells 
have  a  stem  containing  small,  electron-lucent  precursors 
to  secretory  vesicles  proximal  to  the  fluorescent  vesicles. 
Nerve  terminals,  filled  with  large  synaptic  vesicles,  are 
associated  with  the  unsheathed  proximal  cell  membrane. 
Gap  junctions  interconnect  the  nerve  terminals,  and  pos- 
sibly serve  to  accelerate  conduction  to  the  luminous  cell 
to  achieve  a  synchronous  effector  output. 

Introduction 

Many  marine  copepods  produce  brilliant  luminous  se- 
cretions. Despite  many  investigations  (Barnes  and  Case, 
1972;  Herring,  1988;  Bannister  and  Herring,  1989;  Latz 
el  at..  1990),  much  remains  to  be  understood  about  the 
physiology  of  light  production. 

Copepod  luminescence  was  first  thought  to  involve  the 
expulsion  of  luciferin  and  luciferase  from  separate  glands 
through  a  common  pore,  with  mixing  and  light  emission 
occurring  externally  to  the  cuticle  (Clarke  et  al.  1962). 
Recent  studies,  however,  refute  this  theory  (Herring,  1988; 
Bannister  and  Herring,  1989;  Bowlby  and  Case,  1989), 


Received  21  December  1990;  accepted  8  March  1991. 
1  Present  address:  Department  of  Neurobiology.  Harvard  Medical 
School.  220  Longwood  Ave..  Boston.  MA  021 15. 


as  does  this  investigation.  Individual  light  glands  in  some 
Metridinidae  consist  of  a  single  cell  type  occurring  in  a 
unitary  relationship  with  cuticular  pores  (Herring,  1988; 
Bannister  and  Herring,  1989).  There  is  little  evidence, 
other  than  in  some  ostracods,  for  the  separate  cellular 
packaging  of  luciferin  and  luciferase  in  any  luminous  or- 
ganism (Harvey,  1952). 

Control  of  luminous  glands  in  copepods  has  not  pre- 
viously been  investigated,  although  the  short  latency  be- 
tween stimulus  and  light  emission  indicates  a  probable 
nervous  involvement  (Barnes  and  Case,  1972;  Latz  et  a!., 
1987,  1990).  Many  other  organisms,  such  as  crustaceans 
(Dennell,  1940),  teleost  fish  (Nicol,  1 967;  Baguet  and  Case, 
1971;  Anctil  and  Case,  1977),  fireflies  (Buck  and  Case, 
1961;  Case  and  Buck,  1963;  Smith,  1963:  Linberg  and 
Case,  1982),  coelenterates  (Anderson  and  Case,  1975; 
Bassot  et  al..  1978),  and  annelids  (Herrera,  1977),  possess 
demonstrated  or  suspected  neuronal  control  pathways  of 
luminous  glands  (reviewed  by  Case  and  Strause,  1978). 
Experiments  on  euphausiids  suggest  that  serotonin  may 
be  involved  in  neurotransmission  to  the  photophores 
(Kay,  1965,  1966).  The  ophiuroids  have  undergone  an 
extreme  specialization  of  generating  and  propagating  lu- 
minescence within  modified  nerve  cells  of  the  radial  nerve 
cord(Brehm,  1977). 

Luminous  cells  in  copepods  fluoresce  when  excited  with 
ultraviolet  (UV)  light  (Barnes  and  Case,  1972;  Herring, 
1988;  Bannister  and  Herring,  1989),  due  to  absorption  by 
luciferin  and  subsequent  re-emission  in  the  visible  region 
of  the  spectrum.  This  technique,  in  conjunction  with  im- 
age intensification  of  luminous  sites,  has  allowed  identi- 
fication of  at  least  14  luminous  sites  on  the  antennae, 
cephalothorax,  thorax,  mandibular  palps,  and  urosome 
o^G. princeps  (Clarke?/  al..  1962;  Barnes  and  Case,  1972; 
Bannister  and  Herring,  1989;  Bowlby  and  Case,  in  press), 
along  with  a  similar  distribution  of  sites  in  Pleuromamma 


440 


COPEPOD  BIOLUMINESCENCE 


441 


xiphias  and  Metridia  princeps  (Bannister  and  Herring, 
1989). 

The  mesopelagic  calanoid  copepod  G.  princeps  (T. 
Scott)  occurs  below  400  m  during  the  day.  and  vertically 
migrates  to  an  upper  limit  of  200  m  at  night.  They  occur 
in  numbers  up  to  approximately  25  individuals  •  1000  rrT3 
(Childress.  1977). 

In  this  investigation,  the  physiology  of  light  production 
was  studied  in  the  mesopelagic  copepod  Gaussia  princeps. 
Light,  scanning,  and  transmission  electron  microscopy 
were  used  to  elucidate  luminous  cell  ultrastructure  and 
associated  neuroeffector  junctions.  This  may  ultimately 
lead  to  a  more  complete  understanding  of  the  adaptive 
significance  bioluminescence  serves  in  the  midwater  en- 
vironment. 

Materials  and  Methods 

Specimen  collection 

Adult  male  and  female  specimens  of  Gaussia  princeps 
(T.  Scott)  (mean  total  body  length  1  cm)  were  collected 
from  1986  through  1989  from  the  San  Clemente  Basin, 
off  the  coast  of  California,  at  approximately  32°N,  1 1 7°W. 
Collections  were  made  from  the  R.  V.  New  Horizon  and 
R.  V.  Point  Sur,  with  an  opening-closing  Tucker  trawl 
(length,  30  m;  mouth,  10  m2).  The  trawl  was  equipped 
with  an  insulated  cod  end  (Childress  el  a!.,  1978).  and 
towed  between  400  and  800  m  depth.  Specimens  were 
sorted  and  maintained  in  filtered  seawater  at  6°C  until 
testing.  Animals  were  fed  Anemia  nauplii  or  an  unsorted 
zooplankton/phytoplankton  mixture,  collected  locally  at 
10  m  depth,  twice  per  week.  Individuals  survived  for  up 
to  6  months  in  this  regime. 

Pin  'siological  experiments 

Adult  Gaussia  princeps  were  anesthetized  with  2-phen- 
oxyethanol  and  held  non-invasively,  using  fine  U-shaped 
pins,  in  a  Sylgard-lined  petri  dish  filled  with  chilled  sea- 
water.  Specimens  remained  dark  and  undisturbed  for  4 
h,  to  allow  recovery  from  the  anesthetic  and  partial  res- 
toration of  bioluminescent  reserves.  The  abdomen  was 
subsequently  isolated  by  bisection  at  abdominal  segment 
1  ( A 1 ),  and  used  for  all  subsequent  trials.  Secondary  lon- 
gitudinal incisions  were  often  performed,  to  permit  lo- 
calized stimulation  of  different  tissues.  Bioluminescence 
was  elicited  with  single  10-70  V  square  wave  pulses  of  1- 
100  ms  duration,  using  a  5  to  10  Mohm  resistance  tung- 
sten microelectrode  and  an  indifferent  bath  electrode. 
Luminescence  was  recorded  with  a  photomultiplier  tube 
(PMT)  with  a  5  mm  diameter  input  fiber  optic  attached 
to  a  micromanipulator.  The  PMT  signal  was  recorded 
and  stored  on  a  Nicolet  digital  oscilloscope.  Radiometric 
calibration  was  not  performed,  owing  to  the  variable  input 
geometry  of  the  manipulated  fiber  optic. 


Microscopy 

Epifluorescence  microscopy  was  conducted  on  intact 
specimens  and  excised  abdominal  tissue  from  20  anes- 
thetized specimens  using  ultraviolet  light  from  a  mercury 
lamp  filtered  with  365  nm  excitation,  395  nm  dichroic, 
and  420  nm  barrier  filters.  LUtraviolet  and  broadband  vis- 
ible light  were  separately  or  simultaneously  used. 

Primary  fixation  for  light  microscopy,  transmission 
electron  microscopy  (TEM),  and  scanning  electron  mi- 
croscopy (SEM)  was  done  in  1%  paraformaldehyde  and 
3%  glutaraldehyde  in  0.2  A/ sodium  phosphate  buffer  with 
5%  glucose.  Secondary  fixation  was  carried  out  in  2%  OsO4 


1  2  3 

Time  (sec) 


Figure  1.  Bioluminescence  produced  by  excised  Gaussia  princeps 
abdominal  preparations.  The  ventral  nerve  cord  at  abdominal  segment 
1  was  electrically  stimulated  with  single  10-70  V  square  wave  pulses. 
(A|  Photomultiplier  record  of  a  slow  flash.  Mean  flash  latency  is  109  ms. 
(B)  Image  intensified  video  frame  of  the  abdominal  anal  segment  papilla 
and  caudal  rami  luminous  cells.  Luminescence  is  produced  exclusively 
external  to  the  cuticle.  Scale  =  100  jjm.  ap,  anal  papilla;  cr.  caudal  rami; 
lu.  luminous  secretion. 


442 


M.  R    BOWT.BY  AND  J.  F.  CASE 


Figure  2.  Structure  of  Gaussia  princeps  luminous  glands.  (A)  Epifluorescence  microscopy  of  luminous 
cells.  The  anal  papilla  and  caudal  rami  each  contain  three  luminous  cells  filled  with  fluorescent  secretory 
vesicles.  The  third  luminous  cell  is  positioned  below  the  visible  cells.  Scale  =  300  p.m.  (B|  Solitary  fluorescent 
luminous  cells  located  on  the  cephalothorax.  Cells  are  much  smaller  than  those  of  (A),  with  fewer  secretory 
vesicles.  Scale  =  300  jirn.  (C)  SEM  of  the  abdominal  anal  papilla  and  caudal  rami  luminous  cell  cuticular 
pores  (dorsal  view).  Each  structure  contains  three  cuticular  pores.  Pores  not  associated  with  fluorescent/ 
luminescent  sites  occur  on  the  caudal  rami  (unlabeled  arrowhead).  Scale  =  100  nm.  (D)  Luminous  cell 
pores  ( 10^01)  of  one  anal  papilla.  Each  pore  contains  a  closed  valve  in  the  aperture.  Valve  shrinkage  occurred 
in  two  of  the  three  pores.  Scale  =  10  ^m.  ap,  anal  papilla;  ce,  cephalothorax;  cr,  caudal  rami;  p.  pore;  va, 
valve. 


in  0.2  M  sodium  phosphate  buffer.  Fixed  material  was 
rinsed  and  dehydrated  through  an  increasing  ethanol  se- 
ries. Specimens  prepared  for  light  microscopy  and  TEM 
were  transferred  into  propylene  oxide  and  infiltrated  with 
increasing  concentrations  of  Araldite  or  Spurr's  resin  over 
3  days.  Serial  thick  (0.5-1  jum)  and  thin  (0.1  nm)  trans- 
verse and  longitudinal  sections  were  cut  on  a  Sorvall 
Porter-Blum  ultramicrotome  with  glass  knives  for  light 
microscopy  and  TEM.  Light  microscopy  was  performed 
using  a  Zeiss  IM35  inverted  microscope,  while  TEM  was 
done  on  a  Philips  300.  Following  dehydration,  specimens 


prepared  for  SEM  were  critical  point  dried,  sputter  coated 
with  gold-palladium,  and  viewed  with  an  Hitachi  S-4 1 5 A. 
Whole-mount  preparations  of  excised  abdominal  tissue 
were  made  by  primary  fixation  in  the  presence  of  0.1% 
methylene  blue  for  2  h.  Tissues  were  rinsed,  dehydrated, 
and  mounted  on  glass  slides  in  Permount  and  examined 
with  a  Zeiss  IM35  inverted  microscope. 

Image  intensified!  it  »i 

Low  light  level  video  images  of  luminescent  activity  in 
excised  abdominal  preparations  were  made  with  an  ISIT 


COPEPOD  BIOLUMINESCENCE 


443 


V~ 

A  cr 


'J  m, 

I  n 


Figure  3.  Light  microscopy  of  abdominal  luminous  cell  structure.  Total  luminous  cell  length  is  ap- 
proximately 500  Aim.  (A)  Abdominal  whole  mount  stained  with  0.1%  methylene  blue.  Note  the  neural 
process  terminating  on  the  luminous  cell  proximal  stem.  Scale  =  100  Mm.  (B)  Longitudinal  section  through 
the  anal  papilla.  Two  luminous  cells,  filled  with  secretory  vesicles  and  leading  to  separate  cuticular  pores, 
are  shown.  Luminous  cell  nucleus  is  located  at  the  proximal  border  of  the  secretory  vesicles.  A  sheath 
encloses  the  luminous  cell.  Scale  =  50  Aim.  ap,  anal  papilla;  ct,  cuticle;  cr,  caudal  rami;  p,  pore;  ps,  proximal 
stem;  n.  nerve;  nu,  nucleus;  s,  sheath;  sv.  secretory  vesicles. 


video  camera,  an  F/0.95  lens,  and  a  Zeiss  IM35  inverted 
microscope.  Video  images  were  viewed  at  slow  speed  to 
analyze  the  luminescent  patterns,  and  enhanced  with  a 
Megavision  1024XM  image  analysis  system  for  final  pre- 
sentation. Bioluminescence  was  elicited  using  50  V,  100 
ms  square  pulses  delivered  through  tungsten  glass  insu- 
lated microelectrodes. 

Results 

Physiology 

Focal  electrical  stimulation  for  100  ms  at  10  V  near 
the  ventral  nerve  cord  of  the  bisected  abdomen  induced 
luminescence  from  the  caudal  rami  and  anal  segment  pa- 
pillae. (Fig.  1A).  Luminescence  appeared  predominantly 
as  a  slow  flash  with  a  mean  duration  of  3  s  and  a  mean 
latency  of  109  ms.  Nerve  cord  involvement  was  confirmed 
by  stimulation  at  70  V  of  adjacent  longitudinal  muscle 
groups  without  eliciting  luminescence.  Muscle  tissues  were 
also  separated  from  the  ventral  nerve  cord  by  longitudinal 
incisions  from  segments  Al  to  A3.  Stimulation  near  the 
ventral  nerve  cord  in  such  preparations  continued  to  elicit 
luminescence,  while  muscle  stimulation  remained  inef- 
fective. 

The  excised  abdomen  produced  luminescence  only  ex- 
ternal to  the  cuticle  (Fig.  IB).  Light  was  never  emitted 
intracellularly  within  luminous  cells.  Light  appeared  as  a 
localized  glow  near  the  cuticular  pores  of  the  luminous 
cells.  Details  of  excitation  behavior  in  intact  specimens 
are  presented  elsewhere  (Bowlby  and  Case,  in  press). 

Luminous  cell  structure 

Epifluorescence  microscopy  revealed  spherical,  green 
fluorescent,  secretory  vesicles  within  each  luminous  cell 


(Fig.  2A,  B).  Three  cells  occur  in  each  anal  segment  papilla 
and  caudal  ramus  (Fig.  2A),  while  solitary  luminous  cells 
occur  in  the  cephalothorax  (Fig.  2B),  thorax,  basal  8  an- 
tennule  segments,  and  mandibular  palps.  Secretory  ves- 
icles were  visually  confirmed  to  be  discharged  through 
associated  cuticular  pores  in  several  specimens.  It  was  un- 
clear if  secretory  vesicles  were  discharged  intact  or  if  se- 
cretion involved  vesicle  membrane  lysis. 

Three  pores  (10  ^m)  are  located  on  each  anal  papilla 
and  caudal  ramus  (Fig.  2C)  corresponding  to  discharge 
sites  of  the  fluorescent  secretory  vesicles.  Single  pores  are 
located  near  the  fluorescent/luminescent  sites  on  the 
thorax,  cephalothorax,  mandibular  palps,  and  antennule 
segments.  A  closed  valve-like  structure  is  located  in  the 
aperture  of  each  cuticular  pore  (Fig.  2D).  Pore  size  and 
valve  morphology  were  similar  for  luminous  cells  on  the 
cephalothorax  and  thorax.  Some  valves  appear  as  a  par- 
tition dividing  the  pore  aperture  rather  than  as  a  valve, 
although  it  is  suspected  this  is  due  to  asymmetrical 
shrinkage  of  the  valve  away  from  the  cuticle. 

Abdominal  luminous  glands  consist  of  a  single  long 
cell  (approximately  500  ^m),  containing  secretory  vesicles 
distally,  a  nucleus  at  the  proximal  margin  of  the  secretory 
vesicles,  and  a  long  stem  proximal  to  the  nucleus  (Fig. 
3A,  B).  Long  nerve  processes  projecting  from  the  midline 
of  the  specimen  are  associated  with  luminous  cells 
(Fig.  3A). 

Distal  to  the  nucleus,  large  (4  ^m)  secretory  vesicles 
have  amorphous  contents  (Fig.  4A,  B).  Endoplasmic  re- 
ticulum  and  mitochondria  closely  surround  the  secretory 
vesicles.  A  cellular  sheath  surrounds  all  except  the  prox- 
imal end  of  the  luminous  cell  (Figs.  3B;  4A,  B).  This  sheath 
consists  of  layers  of  cells  with  clear  cytoplasm,  whose 


444 


M    R    BOWIBY   AND  J.   F.  CASE 


B 


Figure  4.  TEM  of  luminous  cells.  (A)  Transverse  section  of  an  abdominal  luminous  cell  distal  to  the 
nucleus.  Secretory  vesicles  (4  jjm),  containing  an  amorphous  material,  are  surrounded  by  endoplasmic 
reticulum.  Scale  =  2  ^m.  (B)  Transverse  section  of  the  peripheral  components  of  the  luminous  cell.  The 
luminous  cell  is  surrounded  by  a  sheath  consisting  of  layers  of  thin  cells.  Note  mitochondria  within  the 
endoplasmic  reticulum.  Scale  =  1  pm.  (C)  Proximal  stem  near  the  proximal  limit  of  the  luminous  cell, 
containing  endoplasmic  reticulum  and  2  ^m  electron-lucent  precursors  to  secretory  vesicles,  but  no  cellular 
sheath.  Nerve  terminals  tilled  with  synaptic  vesicles  are  associated  with  the  luminous  cell  membrane.  Scale 
=  3  Mm.  (D)  Nerve  terminals  containing  1 10  nm  synaptic  vesicles  and  interconnected  by  5  nm  gap  junctions 
(arrowheads).  Scale  =  0.5  nm.  cm.  cell  membrane;  er.  endoplasmic  reticulum;  n,  nerve;  s,  sheath;  sv,  secretory 
vesicles. 


membranes  are  separated  by  a  thin  extracellular  space 
containing  fibrous  material  (Fig.  4B). 

The  proximal  limit  of  the  luminous  cell  consists  of  en- 
doplasmic reticulum  and  smaller  (2  ^m)  electron-lucent 
precursors  to  secretory  vesicles,  but  no  cellular  sheath  (Fig. 
4C).  Nerves  are  associated  only  with  this  region  of  the 
luminous  cell.  Nerves  often  contain  large  light  and  dense 
staining  synaptic  vesicles,  and  appear  in  close  association 
with  the  cell  membrane  (Fig.  4C,  D).  Synaptic  vesicles 
are  either  ovoid  (mean  largest  diameter  =  97  nm)  or 
spherical  (mean  diameter  =116  nm).  Nerve  cells  are  in- 
terconnected in  several  locations  by  5  nm  gap  junctions 
(Fig.  4D). 

A  composite  drawing  of  a  hypothetical  Gaussia  princeps 
luminous  cell  indicates  the  predominance  of  secretory 
vesicles  in  the  distal  region  (Fig.  5).  Secretory  vesicles  are 


expelled  through  a  valve  in  the  pore,  which  have  been 
observed  open  or  closed  in  other  genera  (Herring,  1988; 
Bannister  and  Herring,  1989).  The  valve  appears  to  be 
formed  from  the  sheath,  which  surrounds  all  except  the 
proximal  region  of  the  luminous  cell.  Nerve  terminals, 
filled  with  synaptic  vesicles,  are  associated  with  the  prox- 
imal, unsheathed  portion  of  the  cell. 

Discussion 

Luminescence  in  Gaussia  princeps  occurs  by  expulsion 
of  secretory  vesicles  through  a  cuticular  pore.  The  lumi- 
nescent process  presumably  involves  at  least  three  steps: 
(1)  neural  activation  of  the  luminous  cell,  (2)  expulsion 
of  secretory  vesicles  through  cuticular  pores,  culminating 
in  (3)  initiation  of  swimming  movements  and  displace- 


COPEPOD  BIOLUMINESCENCE 


445 


Figure  5.  (/'</».««;  /inmv/iv  Diagrammatic  illustration  of  a  luminous 
cell.  (A)  Longitudinal  view  of  all  portions  of  the  cell.  (B)  Transverse  view 
of  the  secretory  vesicle  containing  portion  of  the  cell.  (C)  and  (D)  Trans- 
verse views  of  the  proximal  stem,  cm,  cell  membrane:  ct.  cuticle;  er. 
endoplasmic  reticulum:  n.  nerves;  nu.  nucleus;  p,  pore;  s.  sheath;  sv. 
secretory  vesicles. 


ment  of  the  animal  away  from  the  persistently  luminous 
bolus. 

Luminous  cells  consist  largely  of  secretory  vesicles 
contained  within  an  endoplasmic  reticulum  matrix.  Pre- 
cursors to  secretory  vesicles  may  be  produced  in  the  prox- 
imal stem  of  the  luminous  cell  and  transported  to  the 
distal  region  of  the  cell,  where  they  fuse  and  acquire 
amorphous  material,  forming  large  secretory  vesicles.  The 
single  luminous  cell  type  of  G.  princeps  appears  similar 
to  those  in  other  members  of  the  Metridinidae  (Herring, 
1988:  Bannister  and  Herring,  1989).  Pleuromammaxiph- 
ias  and  Afetridia  princeps  have  luminous  cells  filled  dis- 
tally  with  secretory  vesicles  and  enclosed  within  a  sheath. 
In  the  Augaptilidae,  in  contrast,  a  pair  of  luminous  cells 
discharge  through  a  common  pore  (Bannister  and  Herring, 
1989).  Secretory  vesicles  are  morphologically  very  differ- 
ent from  the  Metridinidae,  containing  paracrystalline 
contents. 

Fluorescence  from  the  secretory  vesicles  indicates  the 
probable  presence  of  luciferin  or  its  precursors.  In  the 
absence  of  a  second  type  of  differentially  staining  intra- 


cellular  organelles,  the  secretory  vesicles  also  presumably 
contain,  or  have  associated  with  them,  luciferase.  The  ab- 
sence of  necessary  cofactor(s)  such  as  oxygen,  ATP,  Ca+2, 
Mg+2,  or  CV:  (Campbell,  1988,  1989),  may  prevent  lu- 
minescence from  being  expressed  within  the  luminous 
cell.  Many  of  these  cofactor(s)  are  likely  to  be  present  in 
the  seawater,  and  thus  could  initiate  light  generation  once 
expulsion  occurs.  The  slow  decay  of  light  when  secretory 
vesicles  are  not  dispersed  by  appendage  movement  may 
result  from  the  slow  diffusion  of  cofactor(s)  into  the  bolus 
of  secreted  material.  Similarly,  luminescence  induced  by 
mechanical  pressure  on  a  light  gland  may  be  due  to  rup- 
tures of  the  luminous  cell  and  cuticle,  allowing  entry  of 
seawater  and  cofactor(s)  into  the  gland  (Barnes  and  Case, 
1972;  Bannister  and  Herring,  1989).  Alternatively, 
changes  in  permeability  or  breakdown  of  vesicular 
bounding  membrane  upon  excretion  may  permit  the  lu- 
minescence reaction  to  proceed. 

Neuroeffector  junctions  occur  only  on  the  proximal 
region  of  the  luminous  cell.  While  classical  membrane 
active  zones  were  not  found,  large  synaptic  vesicles  fill 
the  terminals.  Gap  junctions  among  peripheral  nerves 
may  serve  to  ephaptically  accelerate  conduction  to  the 
luminous  cell  and  achieve  simultaneous  effector  output. 
The  mechanism  of  material  expulsion  from  the  luminous 
cells  cannot  be  related  to  the  action  of  muscle  fibers  or 
microfilaments,  due  to  their  absence  near  the  cells  (Her- 
ring, 1988;  Bannister  and  Herring,  1989;  present  study). 
Because  luminous  expulsion  occurs  from  excised  abdo- 
mens, changes  in  coelomic  hydrostatic  pressure  are  also 
not  responsible  for  the  expulsion  process.  Release  of  neu- 
rotransmitter  from  the  neuroeffector  junction  synaptic 
vesicles  in  response  to  neural  stimulation  is  assumed  to 
cause  changes  in  luminous  cell  membrane  conductance, 
leading  to  ionic  changes  within  the  cell  and  luminescent 
vesicle  release  to  the  exterior. 

Electrically  induced  flashes  from  excised  abdominal 
preparations  were  predominantly  of  the  slow  flash  type 
described  in  Bowlby  and  Case  (in  press).  The  latent  period, 
however,  is  10  times  shorter  in  excised  tissue  than  in  intact 
specimens.  This  difference  may  be  due  to  the  absence  of 
receptor  delay,  central  nervous  system  processing,  and  to 
a  shorter  final  motor  pathway.  The  absence  of  the  other 
flash  types  in  excised  preparations  is  not  due  to  incapacity 
on  the  part  of  the  abdominal  photocytes,  as  they  produce 
fast,  long,  and  compound  responses  in  intact  specimens. 
Rather,  we  suspect  that  central  nervous  system  temporal 
patterning  of  command  motor  impulses  may  regulate  flash 
patterns. 

Copepods  generally  live  under  conditions  of  high  ki- 
nematic viscosity  and  low  Reynolds  numbers  (Vogel, 
1981).  The  ejection  of  luminous  material  beyond  the 
boundary  layer  of  the  integument  presents  a  problem  un- 
der these  conditions.  Experiments  with  scale  copepod 


446 


M.  R.  BOWLBY  AND  J.  F.  CASE 


models  in  flow  tanks  indicate  that  an  artificial  secretion 
of  a  short  pressure  pulse  produces  a  torus  of  secretory 
product  that  escapes  from  the  boundary  layer  (Herring, 
1988).  Luminous  cells  in  excised  G.  princeps  abdomens 
were  able  to  eject  luminescent  material  beyond  the  cutic- 
ular  boundary  layer.  Thus  luminescent  material  is  ap- 
parently secreted  from  the  abdominal  luminous  cells  as 
a  short  pressure  pulse.  Intact  specimens  also  commonly 
flex  the  abdomen  and  burst/escape  swim  to  further  eject 
luminous  material  and  displace  themselves  from  the  lu- 
minous secretion  (Bowlby  and  Case,  in  press). 

Acknowledgments 

The  authors  wish  to  thank  the  captains  and  crews  of 
the  R.  V.  New  Horizon  and  R.  V.  Point  Sur.  J.  Childress 
generously  provided  ship  time,  and  M.  Latz,  A.  Grutter, 
and  K.  Linberg  provided  technical  assistance.  Supported 
by  the  Office  of  Naval  Research  (Contracts  NOOO 14-84- 
K-0314  and  N00014-87-K-0044). 

Literature  Cited 

Anctil,  M.,  and  J.  F.  Case.  1977.     The  caudal  luminous  organs  of  lantern 

fishes:  general  innervation  and  ultrastructure.  Am.  J  Anal.  149:  1- 

22 
Anderson,  P.  A.  V.,  and  J.  F.  Case.  1975.     Electrical  activity  associated 

with  luminescence  and  other  colonial  behavior  in  the  pennatulid 

Rfirilla  kollikeria.  Biol.  Bull.  149:  80-95. 
Baguet,  F.,  and  J.  Case.  1971.     Luminescent  control  in  Porichthys  (Te- 

leostei):  excitation  of  isolated  photophores.  Biol.  Bull.  140:  15-27. 
Bannister,  N.  J.,  and  P.  J.  Herring.  1989.     Distribution  and  structure 

of  luminous  cells  in  four  marine  copepods.  J.  Mar.  Biol.  Assoc.  UK 

69:  523-533. 
Barnes,  A.  T.,  and  J.  F.  Case.  1972.     Bioluminescence  in  the  mesopelagic 

copepod.  Gaussia  primvps  (T .  Scott).  J.  Exp.  Mar  Biol.  Ecol.  8:  53- 

71. 
Bassot,  J.  M.,  A.  Bilbaut.  G.  O.  Mackie.  L.  M.  Passano,  and  M.  Pavans 

De  Ceccatty.  1978.     Bioluminescence  and  other  responses  spread 

by  epithelial  conduction  in  the  siphonophore  Hippopodius.  Biol.  Bull. 

155:  473-479. 
Bowlby,  M.  R.,  and  J.  F.  Case.  1989.     Endogenous  bioluminescent 

characteristics  of  a  mesopelagic  copepod.  Am   /.on/.  29:  37A.  (Ab- 
stract) 
Bowlby,  M.  R.,  and  J.  F.  Case,  in  press.     Flash  kinetics  and  spatial 

patterns  of  bioluminescence  in  the  copepod  Gaussia  princeps.  Mar. 

Biol. 


Brehm,  P.  1977.     Electrophysiology  and  luminescence  of  an  ophiuroid 

radial  nerve.  J.  Exp.  Biol.  71:  213-227. 
Buck,  J.,  and  J.  F.  Case.  1961.     Control  of  flashing  in  fireflies.  I.  The 

lantern  as  a  neuroeffector  organ.  Biol  Bull.  121:  234-256. 
Campbell,  A.  K.  1988.     Chemiluminescence:  Principles  and  Application 

in  Btologv  ami  Medicine.  VCH/Horwood,  Chichester. 
Campbell,   A.    K.    1989.     Living  light:   biochemistry,   function   and 
biomedical  applications.  Essays  Biochem  (Biochemical  Soc.,  Lond.) 
24:41-81. 
Case,  J.  F.,  and  J.  Buck.  1963.     Control  of  flashing  in  fireflies.  II.  Role 

of  central  nervous  system.  Biol  Bull.  125:  234-250. 
Case,  J.  F.,  and  L.  G.  Strause.  1978.     Neurally  controlled  luminescent 
systems.  Pp.  331-366  in  Bioluminescence  in  Action.  P.  J.  Herring, 
ed.  Academic  Press,  New  York. 

Childress,  J.  J.  1977.  Effects  of  pressure,  temperature  and  oxygen  on 
the  oxygen-consumption  rate  of  the  midwater  copepod  Gaussia  prin- 
ceps. Mar.  Biol.  39:  19-24. 

Childress,  J.  J.,  A.  T.  Barnes,  L.  B.  Quetin,  and  B.  H.  Robison. 
1978.  Thermally  protecting  cod  ends  for  the  recovery  of  living  deep- 
sea  animals.  Deep-Sea  Res.  25:  419-422. 

Clarke,  G.  L.,  R.  Conover,  C.  David,  and  J.  Nicol.  1962.     Comparative 
studies  of  luminescence  in  copepods  and  other  pelagic  marine  animals. 
/  Mar  Biol.  Assoc.  UK  42:  541-564. 
Dennell,  R.  1940.     On  the  structure  of  the  photophores  of  some  decapod 

Crustacea.  Dixcovery  Rep  20:  307-382. 

Harvey,  E.  N.  1952.     Bioluminescence.  Academic  Press.  New  York. 
Herrera,  A.  A.  1977.     The  physiology  of  bioluminescence  in  polynoid 

worms.  Ph.D.  Dissertation,  University  of  California,  Los  Angeles. 
Herring,  P.  J.  1988.     Copepod  luminescence.  Hydrobiologia  167/168: 

183-195. 

Kay,  R.  H.  1965.  Light-stimulated  and  light-inhibited  bioluminescence 
of  the  euphausid  Meganyctipharies  norvegica  (G.  O.  Sars).  Proc.  R. 
Soc.  Lond.  (B)  162:  365-386. 

Kay,  R.  H.  1966.  The  inhibition  of  optically  stimulated  bioluminescence 
in  Meganyctiphanes  narvegica.  George  Allen  and  Unwin  Ltd.,  Lon- 
don. 

Latz,  M.  I.,  M.  R.  Bowlby,  and  J.  F.  Case.  1990.  Recovery  and  stim- 
ulation of  copepod  bioluminescence.  J.  Exp.  Mar.  Biol.  Ecol.  136: 
1-22. 

Latz,  M.  I.,  T.  M.  Frank,  M.  R.  Bowlby,  E.  A.  Widder,  and  J.  F.  Case. 
1987.  Variability  in  flash  characteristics  of  a  bioluminescent  co- 
pepod. Biol.  Bull.  173:  489-503. 

Linberg,  K.,  and  J.  F.  Case.  1982.     Comparative  ultrastructure  of  adult 
light  organs  in  synchronously  and  asynchronously  flashing  fireflies. 
MA.  Thesis,  University  of  California,  Santa  Barbara. 
Nicol,  J.  A.  C.  1967.     The  luminescence  offish.  Symp.  Zoo/.  Soc.  Lond. 

19:27-55. 

Smith,  D.  S.  1963.  The  organization  and  innervation  of  the  luminescent 
organ  in  a  firefly.  Phoiuris  pennsylvanica  (Coleoptera).  J  Cell  Biol. 
16:  323-359. 

Vogel,  S.  1981.  Lite  in  Moving  Fluids.  The  University  Press.  Princeton. 
NJ. 


Reference:  Biol  Bull  180:  447-452.  (June. 


Short-Term  Metallothionein  and  Copper  Changes 
in  Blue  Crabs  at  Ecdysis 


DAVID  W.  ENGEL1  AND  MARIUS  BROUWER2 

1  National  Marine  Fisheries  Sen'ice.  NOAA.  Southeast  Fisheries  Center,  Beaufort  Laboratory. 

Beaufort,  North  Carolina  285] 6,  and  Duke  University  Marine  Laboratory. 

Marine  Biomedical  Center.  Beaufort.  North  Carolina  28516 


Abstract.  We  have  previously  demonstrated  that  the 
small  metal-binding  protein,  metallothionein  (MT),  plays 
an  important  role  in  the  metabolism  of  Cu  and  Zn  during 
the  molt  cycle  of  the  blue  crab,  Callinectes  sapidus.  To 
further  delineate  the  role  of  MT  in  the  regulation  of  both 
metals,  the  distribution  of  copper  and  zinc  was  examined 
immediately  after  ecdysis  in  the  blue  crab.  Hemolymph. 
digestive  gland,  and  stomach  were  analyzed,  by  atomic 
absorption  spectrophotometry  (A AS),  for  total  metal 
concentration  in  crabs  at  different  molt  stages,  from  pre- 
molt  (D,)  through  paper  shell  (B:),  and  including  inter- 
molt  (C4).  Cytosolic  extracts  were  prepared  from  digestive 
glands  of  individual  crabs  and  analyzed,  by  gel  nitration 
chromatography  and  AAS,  for  MT,  copper,  and  zinc.  The 
short-term  changes  in  metal  concentrations  in  the  tissues, 
and  those  in  MT  and  metals  in  the  cytosol  were  dramatic. 
Transient  changes  in  the  metals  bound  to  MT  correlated 
well  with  the  loss  of  copper  from  the  hemolymph  and  the 
digestive  gland.  The  observed  changes  occurred  over  a 
period  of  90  min  after  ecdysis.  The  data  suggest  that  cop- 
per is  stripped  from  hemocyanin  in  the  digestive  gland 
after  ecdysis,  displacing  zinc  from  MT  in  the  cytosolic 
pool.  We  hypothesize  that  the  copper/zinc-MT  complex 
may  then  be  sequestered  in  lysosomes  and  eliminated  into 
the  gut  and  out  in  the  feces.  A  discriptive  flow  model 
showing  the  involvement  of  MT  in  copper  and  zinc  par- 
titioning after  ecdysis  in  the  blue  crab  has  been  con- 
structed. 

Introduction 

Recent  investigations  have  demonstrated  that  molting 
in  the  blue  crab,  Callinectes  sapidus,  profoundly  affects 

Received  22  August  1990;  accepted  27  December  1990. 


the  tissue  and  cytosolic  concentrations  and  partitioning 
of  copper  and  zinc  (Engel,  1987;  Engel  and  Brouwer, 
1987).  At  molt  the  concentration  of  circulating  hemo- 
cyanin, the  copper-containing  respiratory  protein  of  crus- 
taceans, decreases  dramatically.  In  the  blue  crab  this  de- 
crease is  about  60%  (Mangum  et  ai.  1985;  Engel,  1987). 
Because  hemocyanin  is  a  large  copper-containing  protein 
that  occurs  in  high  concentrations  (~50  mg/ml  corre- 
sponding to  0.67  mM  protein  and  1.33  mM  of  copper), 
its  degradation  releases  significant  amounts  of  copper  into 
the  cytosolic  metal  pools.  The  rapidity  of  the  events,  and 
the  reactivity  of  the  copper,  dictate  that  some  mechanism 
must  be  present  to  detoxify  the  copper  and  to  assist  in 
the  excretion  or  storage  of  the  metal.  In  our  earlier  in- 
vestigations, we  have  attempted  to  account  for  the  fate  of 
the  released  copper,  but  have  been  unable  to  find  any 
pool  of  the  metal  stored  in  the  tissues  of  animals  in  the 
papershell  or  early  hard  crab  stages.  These  results  suggested 
that  the  excess  copper  may  be  excreted. 

The  low  molecular  weight  metal-binding  protein,  me- 
tallothionein (MT),  also  changes  in  concentration  and  in 
metal  composition  in  relation  to  the  molt  cycle  (Engel, 
1987;  Engel  and  Brouwer,  1987).  The  changes  that  have 
been  observed  in  MT  correlate  with  the  metabolic  re- 
quirements for  copper  and  the  synthesis  and  turnover  of 
hemocyanin  (Engel  and  Brouwer,  1987;  Brouwer  et  al., 
1989).  Cu-MT  from  marine  crustaceans  can  be  separated 
into  three  different  forms  ( Brouwer  et  ai,  1986).  The  Cu- 
MT(1)  and  Cu-MT(2)  isoforms  cannot  reactivate  apoh- 
emocyanin  in  vitro  (Brouwer  et  al..  1989).  However,  Cu- 
MT(3),  which  differs  in  amino  acid  composition  from 
MT(  1 )  and  MT(  2 ),  can  serve  as  a  copper  donor  for  apohe- 
mocyanin,  and  can  reconstitute  its  oxygen-binding  func- 
tion (Brouwer  et  al..  1989). 


447 


448 


D.  W.  ENGEL  AND  M.  BROUWER 


We  suspected  that,  during  the  breakdown  of  hemocy- 
anin,  the  liberated  copper  is  bound  to  MT  and  excreted 
from  the  crab.  To  test  this  hypothesis,  we  examined  blue 
crabs  just  prior  to,  during,  and  immediately  after  ecdysis 
to  determine  how  the  copper  is  excreted,  and  to  elucidate 
the  mechanism  and  time  course  of  the  process. 

Materials  and  Methods 

All  of  the  premolt  and  postmolt  crabs  used  in  these 
experiments  were  obtained  from  commercial  blue  crab 
shedding  operations  at  Beaufort,  North  Carolina.  The 
premolt  crabs  were  selected  at  the  site  and  transported  to 
the  laboratory  for  sampling  of  tissues.  Tissue  samples  from 
postmolt  animals  were  collected  on  site  at  different  times 
after  ecdysis.  Hemolymph  samples  were  placed  on  ice, 
and  the  digestive  gland  samples  were  frozen  on  dry  ice. 
The  molt  stages  that  were  sampled  in  this  investigation 
were:  premolt,  D,  and  D4:  soft  crab.  A!  and  A:;  and  pa- 
pershell  B,  and  B;.  The  timed  tissue  samples  were  taken 
from  A,  stage  crabs  at  0,  15,  45,  60,  and  90  min  after 
ecdysis. 

The  concentrations  of  copper  and  zinc  were  determined 
in  samples  of  digestive  gland,  stomach,  and  hemolymph 
from  individual  blue  crabs.  The  hemolymph  samples  were 
collected  by  severing  an  appendage  between  two  joints 
and  collecting  the  hemolymph  in  polyethylene  vials.  He- 
molymph was  analyzed  for  hemocyanin,  copper,  and  zinc 
concentrations  {Engel  and  Brouwer.  1987).  Digestive 
glands  and  stomachs  were  collected  for  total  metal  anal- 
ysis. Portions  of  the  digestive  glands  were  stored  at  -70°C 
for  the  determination  of  the  cytosolic  distribution  of  met- 
als and  metallothioneins. 

Tissue  samples  used  for  metal  analysis  were  dried  at 
100°C  for  24  to  48  h  and  wet  ashed  with  concentrated 
HNO3  at  90°C.  The  residue  was  dissolved  in  0.25  N  HC1, 
and  the  concentrations  of  copper  and  zinc  were  measured 
by  flame  atomic  absorption  spectrophotometry.  Prepar- 
ative and  measurement  techniques  were  calibrated  against 
the  National  Bureau  of  Standards,  Oyster  Reference  Ma- 
terial #1566. 

The  cytosolic  distribution  of  copper,  zinc,  and  MT  was 
determined  by  gel  filtration  chromatography  with  Seph- 
adex  G-75  (Engel,  1987).  In  these  investigations,  a  com- 
puter program  was  developed  that  allows  for  the  averaging 
of  elution  profiles  so  that  there  is  less  subjectivity  in  the 
evaluation  of  results.  The  program  requires  the  use  of 
uniform  methodologies,  and  provides  metal  concentra- 
tions in  each  fraction  in  terms  of  micromoles  of  metal 
per  kilogram  wet  weight  of  tissue. 

Data  on  molt-induced  changes  in  tissue  metal  concen- 
trations were  tested  for  significance  (P  <  0.05)  by  analysis 
of  variance  and  Tukey's  studentized  range  test.  The  cy- 
tosolic distributions  of  copper  and  zinc  were  analyzed  with 


the  assistance  of  a  computer  program  developed  in  our 
Laboratory  that  gave  average  elution  profiles. 

Results 

The  concentrations  of  hemocyanin.  copper,  and  zinc 
in  the  hemolymph,  and  of  copper  and  zinc  in  the  digestive 
gland,  varied  with  molt  stage  (Figs.  1,  2).  Significant 
changes  occurred  throughout  the  molt  cycle,  in  the  level 
of  hemocyanin,  and  in  the  concentrations  of  copper  and 
zinc  in  the  hemolymph  and  digestive  gland  (ANOVA,  P 
<  0.05).  In  the  hemolymph,  concentrations  of  hemocy- 
anin, copper,  and  zinc  decreased  significantly  (P  <  0.05) 
at  ecdysis,  between  D4  and  A, .  and  remained  at  reduced 
levels  throughout  the  papershell  stage.  There  was  some 
indication  that  the  hemocyanin  concentration  was  in- 
creasing at  the  end  of  the  papershell  stage  (Fig.  1 ).  In  the 
digestive  gland,  a  transient,  significant  (P  <  0.05)  increase 
in  copper  concentration  occurred  in  stage  A,  relative  to 
stages  D4  and  A:  (Fig.  2).  This  increase  in  copper  con- 
centration in  the  digestive  gland  coincides  with  the  de- 
creases in  the  concentrations  of  both  hemocyanin  and 
copper  in  the  hemolymph.  The  subsequent  decrease  in 
copper  concentration  in  A:  suggests  that  the  copper  is 
being  lost  from  the  tissue. 

The  transient  increase  in  copper  during  A,  is  not  seen 
with  zinc  (Figs.  1,2).  When  the  changes  in  copper  and 
zinc  in  the  digestive  gland  are  compared  on  a  molar  basis, 
an  increase  of  about  0.3  mA//kg  in  copper  is  revealed 
between  D4  and  A,,  and  a  concomitant  0.3  mM/kg  de- 
crease occurs  in  zinc  (Fig.  2).  However,  this  copper/zinc 
relation  does  not  hold  as  the  crabs  go  from  A,  to  A:.  The 
data  suggest  that,  during  those  stages  of  the  molt  cycle, 
both  copper  and  zinc  are  lost  from  the  digestive  gland. 

To  determine  the  possible  route  of  metal  loss  by  the 
crabs  during  the  period  following  ecdysis,  stomachs  of 
hard  (C4),  soft  ( Ai-A:),  and  papershell  (B,-B:)  crabs  were 
examined  for  concentrations  of  copper  and  zinc  (Fig.  3). 
The  data  from  these  measurements  show  that  the  stom- 
achs of  soft  crabs  have  significantly  higher  concentrations 
of  copper  and  zinc  (P  <  0.05)  than  those  of  either  hard 
crabs  or  papershell  crabs,  which  were  not  significantly  dif- 
ferent. Such  information  supports  the  idea  that  the  path- 
way for  the  loss  of  copper  and  zinc  following  ecdysis 
leads  from  the  digestive  gland  to  the  gut,  and  that  the 
metals  are  excreted  in  the  feces  (e.g.,  digestive  gland  -*• 
stomach  ->•  gut  -*•  feces). 

Because  the  changes  in  copper  and  zinc  concentrations 
in  the  digestive  gland  apparently  occur  quite  rapidly,  short- 
term  measurements  were  made  of  the  cytosolic  partition- 
ing of  copper  and  zinc  (i.e.,  the  portion  of  metals  bound 
to  MT).  Although  the  total  amount  of  copper  changed, 
the  partitioning  of  copper  was  similar  at  each  sampling 
time.  The  maximum  amount  of  copper  was  bound  to  MT 


COPPER,  ZINC,  AND  MT 


449 


D-3  D-4 


fl-1  fl-2 

MOLT   STflGE 


B-2 


fl-1  fl-2 

MOLT  STFGE 


B-l 


B-2 


fl-1  fl-2  B- 

MOLT  STflGE 


B-2 


Figure  1.  Histograms  showing  the  average  concentrations  of  hemocyanin,  copper  and  zinc  in  the  he- 
molymph  at  selected  stages  of  the  molt  cycle.  All  concentrations  of  hemocyanin,  copper  and  zinc  are  reported 
in  millimoles/kilogram  wet  weight  of  sample.  Each  bar  represents  the  mean  of  six  crabs  and  the  vertical 
lines  above  and  below  the  mean  describe  one  standard  error. 


60  min  after  ecdysis  (Fig.  4).  While  the  amount  of  Cu- 
MT  increased  in  the  cytosol  from  1 5  through  60  min  after 
ecdysis,  the  amounts  of  Zn-MT  decreased  over  the  same 
period.  In  the  period  between  45  and  60  min  after  ecdysis, 
there  was  about  a  10  micromolar  increase  in  copper  bound 
to  MT  and  a  similar  decrease  in  the  amount  of  bound 
zinc.  This  observation  suggests  that  zinc  was  displaced 
from  the  MT  by  copper  released  during  the  rapid  degra- 
dation of  hemocyanin  following  ecdysis.  Cytosolic  copper 
concentrations  were  initially  low,  but  had  increased  five 
fold  by  60  min  after  ecdysis  (Fig.  5),  which  correlates  well 
with  the  observed  increase  in  copper  bound  to  MT  (Fig. 
4).  There  was  very  little  change  in  cytosolic  zinc  concen- 
tration for  the  first  60  min  after  ecdysis,  but  it  did  decrease 
between  60  and  90  min.  These  decreases  in  both  Cu  and 
Zn-MT  and  in  total  cytosolic  copper  between  60  and  90 
min  suggests  again  that  metal  was  lost  from  the  digestive 
gland  at  this  time. 


Discussion 

From  the  data  collected  in  our  current  and  earlier  ex- 
periments (Engel,  1987;  Engel  and  Brouwer,  1987)  on  the 
mechanisms  of  copper  and  zinc  metabolism  during  the 
molt  cycle  of  the  blue  crab,  we  have  constructed  a  diagram 
showing  the  relationships  between  the  breakdown  of  he- 
mocyanin and  the  changes  in  the  concentrations  of  Cu- 
MT  and  Zn-MT  (Fig.  6).  Three  significant  changes  occur 
in  the  metals  bound  to  MT  during  the  molt  cycle.  The 
first  is  at  the  beginning  of  premolt  when  the  metals  bound 
to  MT  change  from  predominantly  copper  to  zinc.  The 
second  occurs  within  90  min  after  ecdysis  when  there  is 
a  transient  pulse  of  copper  bound  to  the  predominantly 
zinc-MT.  The  third  change  occurs  during  stages  B,  and 
B2  when  the  MT  once  again  becomes  primarily  a  copper 
protein,  and  this  change  is  correlated  with  synthesis  of 
hemocyanin  (Engel  and  Brouwer,  1987). 


450 


D    W.  ENGEL  AND  M.  BROUWER 


0.0 


D-3 


0-4 


R-l  fl-2 

MOLT   STHGE 


B-2 


D-3 


D-4 


fl-t  fl-2 

MOLT   STflGE 


Figure  2.  Histograms  showing  the  copper  and  zinc  concentrations  in  digestive  glands  of  blue  crabs 
selected  stages  of  the  molt  cycle.  All  concentrations  and  error  designations  are  the  same  as  described 
Figure  1. 


B-2 


At  the  end  of  the  intermolt  stage,  C4 ,  and  the  beginning 
of  premolt,  D,,  the  metal  bound  to  MT  changes  from 
predominantly  copper  to  zinc.  The  trigger  for  this  change 
has  yet  to  be  demonstrated.  We  hypothesize,  however, 
that  the  reduction  in  the  concentrations  of  Cu-MT  is  cor- 
related with  reduced  hemocyanin  synthesis  and  an  in- 
creased rate  of  Zn-carbonic  anhydrase  synthesis  in  prep- 
aration for  molting.  These  types  of  changes,  which  preceed 
molting,  could  be  initiated  by  increases  in  the  concentra- 
tion of  the  molting  hormone,  ecdysteroid  (Soumoffand 
Skinner,  1983).  While  the  magnitude  of  the  changes  in 
the  metals  bound  to  MT  are  large,  from  90%  copper  to 
90%  zinc  (Engel,  1987),  no  information  is  available  on 
either  the  timing  or  rate  of  the  change. 

The  transient  pulse  of  copper  bound  to  MT  in  the 
digestive  gland  cytosol  immediately  after  ecdysis  (i.e.. 
within  90  min)  is  undoubtedly  correlated  with  the  catab- 
olism  of  hemocyanin.  Because  there  is  roughly  a  60%  de- 
crease in  hemocyanin  concentration  in  the  hemolymph 
shortly  after  molt  (Fig.  1)  (Mangum  et  ai.  1985;  Engel, 
1987),  a  large  quantity  of  copper  should  be  released  into 
the  cytosol  of  the  digestive  gland  in  a  relatively  short  time. 
The  observed  pulse  of  Cu-MT,  therefore,  represents  the 
detoxification  of  the  liberated  copper  by  an  in  situ  pro- 
cesses in  the  digestive  gland  cytosol.  To  more  fully  describe 
our  hypothesis,  a  flow  diagram  has  been  developed  that 
shows  the  interaction  between  the  released  copper  and 
the  cytosolic  pool  of  Zn-MT  present  at  ecdysis  (Fig.  7). 
The  mechanism  of  copper  detoxification  may  be  a 
straightforward  substitution  process  involving  the  pool  of 
Zn-MT  already  present  in  the  digestive  gland.  This  large 
concentration  of  Zn-MT  in  the  cytosol  acts  as  a  sink  for 
the  copper  that  is  released  during  the  degradation  of  he- 
mocyanin. Because  copper  has  a  higher  binding  affinity 


for  MT  than  does  zinc,  it  simply  displaces  the  zinc  already 
bound  to  MT.  This  process  would  account  for  the  rapid 
kinetics,  because  de  novo  synthesis  of  MT  is  unlikely  to 
occur  rapidly  (Hildebrand  and  Enger,  1980).  This  substi- 
tution process  will  not  result  in  an  all-copper  protein.  After 
the  substitution,  a  significant  portion  of  the  Cu/Zn-MT 
complex  is  excreted  via  lysosomes  into  the  digestive  tract 
and  out  in  the  feces,  and  the  remainder  may  serve  as  the 
initial  copper  donor  for  renewed  hemocyanin  synthesis. 
The  excess  zinc  not  bound  to  MT  can  either  be  excreted 


go 

E 

3  COPPER    D  ZINC 

80 

T 

CD          70 

I  —  ^ 

(X   1-     _ 

tr  I   60 

t-   CD 

S  in  gp 

0  1- 

<_)  LU   40 

c 

I 

" 

I 

^^ao 

o  ^ 

o 

Z     CD 

CH   ^    ZQ 

r 

| 

CJ 

$s 

-T 

10 

• 

x^ 

^ 

n 

1 

1 

1 

HflRD  CRflB 


MOLT  STflGE 


Figure  3.  Histograms  showing  the  copper  and  zinc  concentrations 
in  the  stomachs  of  hard  crabs  [n  =  8],  soft  crabs  (A,  and  A2)  [n  =  5], 
and  papershell  crabs  (B,  and  B;)  [n  =  6].  Concentrations  are  given  as 
milligrams  of  metal  per  kilogram  of  tissue  plus  and  minus  one  standard 
error  of  the  mean. 


COPPER,  ZINC,  AND  MT 


451 


LU 
—  L    **n 

—  >      OJ 

<f> 

COPPER                                   MINUTES 

I— 

A      0 

CN=2) 

^    IB 

»»«     °  15 

(N=3) 

/    \       G   45 

(N-4) 

tr 

LU 

°-  to 

/      ",      •    60 
/         \    +    9° 

(N  =  4) 
(N-4) 

Q_ 
O 

/      \ 

^~^ 

* 

CD 
LU 
_l      5 

/     0          1 

0 

>                           /rf,/j    °        « 

0 
QL 
CJ 

4  mfl.  ^^k  K 

^      Q 

*—  SA»AA  i.VffffA£L  —  i  iBv^AvAvs 

Md666666Ai 

10        16 


20 


25    X    36   40 

FRRCTIDN  NUMBER 


BO        66 


BO 


Figure  -4.  Average  elution  profiles  of  protein  bound  copper  and  zinc 
in  digestive  gland  cytosols  at  five  specific  times  after  ecdysis  (0,  15,  45, 
60,  and  90  min).  Each  elution  profile  is  a  computer  generated  average 
of  from  2  to  4  individual  crabs.  The  concentrations  of  copper  and  zinc 
are  normalized  to  the  wet  weight  of  the  tissue  used  and  to  the  amount 
of  cytosol  applied  to  the  column. 

in  lysosomes  via  the  gut,  or  via  the  green  gland  in  the 
urine. 

Our  functional  model  of  copper  detoxification  agrees 
with  the  observations  made  by  Al-Mohanna  and  Nott 
( 1989)  on  the  shrimp  Penaeus  semisulcatus.  In  their  elec- 
tron microscopic  examination  of  the  shrimp  hepatopan- 
creas  during  the  molt  cycle,  they  demonstrated  the  pres- 
ence of  copper  and  sulfur  containing  granules  in  the  R 
cells  of  the  hepatopancreas  using  EDAX  energy  dispersive 
microanalysis.  These  granules  are  released  to  the  lumen 
of  the  hepatopancreas  through  cellular  degeneration  and 
sloughing.  The  occurrence  of  these  copper  containing 
granules  may  be  associated  with  the  synthesis  and  turnover 
of  hemocyanin. 

The  conversion  of  the  MT  back  to  a  copper  protein 
occurs  later  in  the  postmolt  period,  B,  and  Bi,  and  pre- 


0.50 
0.45 
0.40 
0.35 
0.30 
0.25 
0.20 
0.15 
10.10 
0.05 
0.00 


D  COPPER     Q  ZINC 


15  45  60 

TIME   HFTER  ECDYSIS   (MINUTES) 


Figure  5.  Histograms  showing  the  average  cytosolic  concentrations 
of  copper  and  zinc  (;  i'.  millimoles/kg)  in  digestive  glands  at  five  times 
after  ecdysis  (0.  15,  45,  60,  and  90  min).  These  metal  concentrations  are 
calculated  from  the  cytosolic  samples  applied  to  the  Sephadex  G-75  col- 


cedes  the  resynthesis  of  hemocyanin  (Engel,  1987;  Engel 
and  Brouwer,  1987),  strongly  suggesting  that  Cu-MT  may 
act,  directly  or  indirectly,  as  the  source  of  copper  for  he- 
mocyanin synthesis  (Fig.  7).  Brouwer  and  coworkers 
(1986,  1989)  have  demonstrated  that  Class-I  Cu-MTs  [i.e., 
related  in  primary  structure  to  equine  renal  MT  (Fowler 
el  ai,  1987)]  isolated  from  the  hepatopancreas  of  the 
American  lobster  cannot  transfer  their  copper  to  hemo- 
cyanin. However,  a  third  copper-protein,  which  has  a 
lower  molecular  weight  than  the  Class-I  MTs,  contains 
less  cysteine,  and  is  much  more  acidic,  has  been  isolated 
from  the  lobster  hepatopancreas.  This  copper-protein. 


Zn-MT 


O 

O 

2 

UJ 

I 


D, 


D3     D4|  A,      A2 
MOLT   STAGE 


B,      B2     C 


Figure  6.  A  descriptive  diagram  showing  the  relationships  between 
hemocyanin  in  the  hemolymph  and  copper  and  zinc  MT  during  the 
molt  cycle  of  the  blue  crab.  The  arrow  denotes  the  time  of  ecdysis. 


452 


D.  W.  ENGEL  AND  M.  BROUWER 


HEHOCYRNIN 


Zn/Cu-MT 


Zn 


HMINO    RCIDS 


ENERGY  METHBOLISM 


LYSOSOMES 

I 


(EXCRETION) 


(  ce  I  I  u  I  or  si  ough  i  ng  ) 


FECES 


URINE 


Figure  7.  A  flow  diagram  of  the  synthetic  and  catabolic  pathways 
tor  hemocyanin  and  the  interactions  between  copper,  zinc,  and  copper/ 
zmc-MT  in  the  digestive  gland  of  the  blue  crab  immediately  after  ecdysis 
and  during  the  later  postmolt  recovery  period.  The  diagram  includes  the 
pathways  for  detoxification  of  copper  released  from  hemocyanin,  excre- 
tion of  copper  and  zinc  (Engel,  1987;  Engel  and  Brouwer,  1987;  and  Al- 
Mohanna  and  Nott.  1989),  and  the  presence  of  a  lower  molecular  weight 
compound  that  is  active  in  the  transfer  of  copper  to  the  apoprotein  during 
hemocyanin  synthesis  (Brouwer,  unpub.  data). 


tentatively  classified  as  a  Class  II-MT,  has  been  found 
effective  in  restoring  the  oxygen  binding  capacity  of  apo- 
hemocyanin  (Brouwer  et  al.  1989).  Whether  copper  ex- 
change occurs  between  the  Class  I  and  II-MTs  remains 
to  be  demonstrated. 

This  investigation  gives  further  support  to  the  hypoth- 
esis that  the  function  of  metallothionein  in  normal  or- 
ganisms is  in  the  regulation  of  nutritional  metals.  Through 
the  use  of  the  normal  crustacean  growth  process  of  molt- 
ing, we  have  been  able  to  identify  some  of  the  functional 
mechanisms  of  cytosolic  metal  regulation  involving  MT. 
These  data  also  serve  to  point  out  that  if  a  protein  such 
as  MT  is  to  be  used  as  an  indicator  of  animal  health,  the 
processes  that  control  its  abundance  and  turnover  must 
be  demonstrated. 


Acknowledgments 

The  authors  thank  Mr.  William  J.  Bowen,  III  for  his 
efforts  to  collect  the  tissue  samples  from  molting  blue  crabs 
at  the  desired  times,  and  the  local  crab  shedders  for  their 
cooperation  (i.e.,  Mr.  Garry  Culpepper,  Hooper  Family 
Seafood,  and  Pitmann  Seafood).  The  authors  also  ac- 
knowledge the  assistance  of  the  ADP/Biometrics  Unit  in 
the  statistical  analysis  of  the  data  and  in  the  development 
of  the  computer  program  for  averaging  elution  profiles. 
This  research  was  supported  by  the  National  Marine 
Fisheries  Service  and  by  the  National  Institutes  of  Health 
Grant  ESO  4074  (M.B.). 

Literature  Cited 

Al-Mohanna,  S.  Y.,  and  J.  A.  Nott.  1989.  The  accumulation  of  metals 
in  the  hepatopancreas  of  the  shnmp  Penaeus  semisulcatus  de  Hann 
(Crustacea;  Decapoda)  during  the  molt  cycle.  Pp.  195-207  in  Pro- 
ceedings of  the  First  Arabian  Conference  on  Environment  and  Pol- 
lution. Allen  Press,  Oxford. 

Brouwer,  M.,  D.  R.  W'inge,  and  W.  R.  Gray.  1989.  Structural  and 
functional  diversity  of  copper-metallothionem  from  the  American 
lobster,  Homarus  americanus.  J.  Inorg.  Biochem  35:  289-303. 

Brouwer,  M.,  P.  Whaling,  and  D.  W.  Engel.  1986.  Copper-metallo- 
thioneins  in  the  American  lobster,  Homarus  americanus:  potential 
role  as  Cu(I)  donors  to  apohemocyanin.  Environ.  Health  Pcrspect. 
65:93-100. 

Engel,  I).  W.  1987.  Metal  regulation  and  molting  in  the  blue  crab, 
Callinectes  sapidus:  copper,  zinc,  and  metallothionein.  Biol.  Bull. 
172:69-82. 

Engel,  D.  \V.,  and  M.  Brouwer.  1987.  Metal  regulation  and  molting 
in  the  blue  crab,  Callinectes  sapidus:  metallothionein  function  in 
metal  metabolism.  Biol.  Bull.  173:  237-249. 

Fowler,  B.  A.,  C.  E.  Hildebrand,  Y.  Kojima,  and  M.  Webb. 
1987.  Nomenclature  of  metallothionein.  Experienlia  Suppl.  52:  19- 

Hildebrand,  C.  E.,  and  M.  D.  Enger.  1980.  Regulation  ofCd-VZn2*- 
stimulated  metallothionein  synthesis  during  induction,  deinduction, 
and  superinduction.  Biochemistry  19:  5850-5857. 

Mangum,  C.  P.,  B.  R.  McMahon,  P.  L.  DeFur,  and  M.  G.  Wheatly. 

1985.  Gas  exchange,  acid-base  balance,  and  the  oxygen  supply  to 
the  tissues  during  a  molt  of  the  blue  crab,  Callinectes  sapidus.  J 
Cms/  Biol.  5:  188-206. 

Soumoff,  C.,  and  D.  M.  Skinner.  1983.  Ecdysteroid  tilers  during  the 
molt  cycle  of  the  blue  crab  resembled  those  of  other  Crustacea.  Biol. 
Bull  165:  321-329. 


Reference:  Biol  Bull  180:  453-465.  (June,  1991) 


Musculature  Associated  with  the  Water  Canals 

in  Freshwater  Mussels  and  Response 

to  Monoamines  In  Vitro 

DAVID  B.  GARDINER,  HAROLD  SILVERMAN,  AND  THOMAS  H.  DIETZ 

Department  of  Zoology  and  Physiology,  Louisiana  State  University.  Baton  Rouge,  Louisiana.  70803 


Abstract.  The  gills  of  freshwater  mussels  perform  many 
functions  that  depend  on  water  flow  through  the  water 
canals  and  channels.  Regulation  of  water  flow  depends 
in  part  on  ciliary  activity  and  in  part  on  the  contraction 
of  musculature  underlying  the  gill  filament  and  water 
channel  epithelium.  Obliquely  striated  muscles  control 
water  canal  openings  (ostia)  at  the  base  of  the  filaments 
and  also  at  the  entry  into  the  water  channel  (internal  ostia, 
IO).  The  muscles  adjacent  to  the  ostia  are  oriented  in  an 
anterior-posterior  direction  (perpendicular  to  gill  fila- 
ments), and  those  controlling  the  internal  ostia  are  ori- 
ented in  a  dorso-ventral  direction  (parallel  to  gill  fila- 
ments). Small  bundles  of  fibers  radiate  from  the  major 
dorso-ventral  IO  muscle  bands  and  appear  to  insert  at  the 
base  of  the  water  canal  epithelial  cells  at  the  canal-channel 
junction.  Both  muscular  bands  are  closely  associated  with 
the  branchial  nerves  in  the  gill.  When  gills  are  exposed  to 
10" 5  M  serotonin  in  vitro,  both  ostial  openings  dilate  and 
gill  ciliary  activity  increases.  The  net  result  of  serotonin 
treatment  is  an  increase  in  ciliary  activity,  a  maximal 
opening  of  the  water  canal  ostia,  and,  presumably,  an 
increase  in  water  flow  through  the  gill. 

Introduction 

The  gill  in  freshwater  mussels  is  responsible  for  many 
of  the  functions  associated  with  water  flow  through  the 
animal.  For  example,  ion  transport,  feeding,  reproduction, 
and  respiration  are  all  dependent  on  the  pattern  of  water 


Received  12  December  1990;  accepted  8  March  1991. 

Abbreviations:  Acetylcholine  (ACh);  Epinephrine  (Epi);  Gamma 
Aminobutyric  Acid  (GABA);  Internal  Ostia  (IO);  Norepinephrine  (Nor- 
epi);  Ostia  (O);  Scanning  Electron  Microscopy  (SEM);  Transmission 
Electron  Microscopy  (TEM) 


flow  through  the  gill.  The  gill  ciliary  activity  generates  the 
force  for  water  flow  (Riisgard  and  Mohlenberg,  1979;  Jor- 
gensen,  1982.  1989;Paparo,  1988;  Silvester,  1988;  Sleigh, 
1989),  and  water  flow  has  been  calculated  from  data  char- 
acterizing ciliary  activity  (Jorgensen  1989;  Sleigh,  1989). 
The  pattern  of  flow  through  the  gill  begins  with  water 
moving  across  the  gill  filaments  and  through  the  ostial 
(O)  openings  that  lead  into  the  water  canals.  From  the 
canals,  water  flow  is  directed  through  the  internal  ostia 
(IO),  into  the  central  water  channels  that  conduct  water 
into  the  suprabranchial  chamber,  and  then  out  through 
the  excurrent  siphon  (see  Fig.  1 ). 

The  specializations  found  in  the  various  gill  epithelia 
indicate  that  ion  transport  and  perhaps  respiration  take 
place  across  the  internal  epithelial  lining  the  water  canals 
and  channels  (Kays  ct  a/..  1990).  The  epithelial  cells  of 
the  gill  showing  the  most  enzymatic  activity  for  carbonic 
anhydrase  are  located  on  the  internal  epithelial  surfaces 
(Kayset  a/.,  1990).  In  addition,  the  cells  showing  the  most 
oxidative  activity  form  the  epithelia  lining  the  canals.  The 
ciliated  epithelia  lining  the  filaments  do  not  appear  to 
contain  any  of  the  specializations  associated  with  ion 
transporting  cells  and  are  larger  (apical  to  basal  surface) 
than  one  would  expect  for  gas  exchange.  They  appear  to 
be  providing  protection,  as  well  as  the  driving  force  for 
water  flow. 

While  ciliary  activity  may  be  the  principal  driving  force 
for  water  flow,  the  pattern  of  flow  may  be  regulated  by 
the  muscles  present  in  the  gill  tissue.  In  oysters,  the  gill 
musculature  and  vascular  changes  control  the  diameter 
of  the  ostia  (Galtsoff,  1964;  Nelson,  1941;  Nelson  and 
Allison,  1940)  and  influence  the  rate  of  water  flow  through 
the  gill  (Nelson,  1941;  Nelson  and  Allison,  1940).  Similar 
regulation  of  ostial  diameter  by  muscles  in  unionid  gills 


453 


454 


D.  B.  GARDINER  KT    \1 


would  control  water  flow  in  response  to  the  osmoregu- 
latory  ( Dietz  and  Graves,  1981),  respiratory,  and  even  the 
reproductive  needs  of  the  animal  (Silverman,  1989;  Rich- 
ard et  al..  1991).  Muscular  control  also  offers  the  possi- 
bility of  blocking  flow  into  the  water  channels.  Previously 
we  showed  that  the  water  channels  of  the  Lampsilinae 
are  functionally  occluded  during  reproduction  (Silverman 
el  al..  1987). 

In  the  research  reported  here,  we  have  used  morpho- 
logical techniques  to  describe  the  musculature  associated 
with  the  water  canals  of  the  freshwater  unionid  gills.  We 
have  also  demonstrated  that  serotonin,  a  well-known  in- 
hibitor of  muscle  contraction  in  a  number  of  molluscan 
systems  (Twarog  1954;  Cambridge,  1959;  Twarog  and 
Cole,  1972;Jorgensen,  1976;  Satchell  and  Twarog,  1978; 
Kobayashi  and  Hasimoto,  1982),  causes  the  canal  mus- 
culature to  relax,  allowing  the  water  canals  to  expand. 
Serotonin  increases  ciliary  activity  in  a  variety  of  marine 
mussels  (Gosselin  et  al..  1962;  Aiello  and  Guideri,  1966; 
Aiello,  1970;  Paparo  and  Murphy,  1975;  Jorgenson,  1976; 
Capatanert  al.,  1978;  Paparo,  1 980;  Sanderson  and  Satir, 
1982;  Sanderson  et  al..  1985),  and  dopamine  depresses 
ciliary  activity  in  marine  bivalves  (Catapane  et  al..  1978; 
Paparo,  1980).  However,  we  report  here  that  both  sero- 
tonin and  dopamine  increase  gill  ciliary  activity  in  the 
freshwater  unionids. 

Materials  and  Methods 

Animal  maintenance 

The  unionid  mussels  Anodonta  grandis  and  Ligumia 
subrostrata  were  collected  from  ponds  near  Baton  Rouge, 
Louisiana.  The  animals  were  maintained  in  aerated  ar- 
tificial pondwater  (0.5  mM NaCl,  0.4  mM CaCl2, 0.2  mM 
NaHCO3,  and  0.05  mM  KC1)  at  25°C  and  were  allowed 
to  acclimate  to  laboratory  conditions  for  a  week  before 
being  used.  The  mussels  were  only  studied  during  the  non- 
reproductive  season  so  the  gills  were  not  being  employed 
to  brood  larvae. 

Preparation  of  gills  for  light  and  transmission  electron 
microscopy 

We  opened  the  clams  by  cutting  the  adductor  muscles, 
thereby  exposing  the  lateral  and  medial  demibranch  (gill) 
pairs.  The  gills  were  excised  and  placed  in  a  Ringer's  so- 
lution designed  for  freshwater  mussels  (5.0  mM  CaCl:, 
0.5  mM  KC1,  5.0  mM  NaCl,  5.0  mM  NaHCO3  and  5.0 
mM  Na2SO4)  or  a  30  mM  tris(hydroxymethyl)amino- 
methane  (tris-HCl)  buffer  solution,  pH  7.8.  After  several 
minutes,  the  gills  were  removed  and  flattened  on  a  poly- 
styrene petri  dish  or  pinned  to  a  wax  base. 

Gills  were  fixed  in  2%  glutaraldehyde  (EM  grade)  in  30 
mM  tris-HCl  containing  1  mA/ethylenediaminetetraace- 


Figure  1.  A  schematic  representation  of  the  gill  of  Ligumia  xuhro- 
xtrata  modified  from  Kays  el  al.  (1990).  The  gill  consists  of  an  ascending 
and  descending  lamella  (L)  organized  as  filaments  (F)  surrounding  central 
water  channels  (WC).  The  lamellae  are  joined  by  connective  tissue  septa 
(S).  The  filaments  are  supported  by  discontinuous  calcified  chitinous 
rods  (R)  and  an  associated  mucopolysaccharide  matrix  (P).  Extensive 
extracellular  calcium  concretions  (cc)  are  located  in  the  connective  tissue 
underlying  the  filaments.  Blood  sinuses  (B)  also  occur  in  this  region. 
Water  enters  the  gill  through  ostia  (O)  located  at  the  base  of  the  filaments. 
The  ostia  open  into  water  canals  (C)  which  lead  into  the  WC.  The  opening 
of  the  water  canal  into  the  WC  is  designated  as  the  internal  ostia  (1O). 
Water  moves  into  the  WC.  and  is  directed  dorsally  to  the  suprabranchial 
chamber.  The  general  direction  of  water  flow  through  the  gill  is  indicated 
by  the  arrows.  Associated  with  the  rods  are  anterior-posterior  oriented 
bands  of  muscle  (NM);  these  bands  are  associated  with  nerve  fibers  which 
are  oriented  in  the  same  direction.  This  musculature  flares  and  inserts 
onto  adjacent  chitinous  rods  at  discontinuities  in  the  rods.  The  muscle 
bands  alternate  with  ostial  openings  at  the  base  of  the  filaments.  Associated 
with  the  internal  ostia  are  bands  of  muscle  (IM)  oriented  in  the  dorsal- 
ventral  direction;  these  bundles  of  muscle  also  are  associated  with  nerve 
fibers.  Water  canal  epithelial  cells  are  non-ciliated  microvillar  cells.  Cil- 
iated cells  (CI)  are  oriented  in  rows  in  the  water  channel;  the  epithelial 
cells  forming  the  border  of  the  IO  are  also  ciliated  (not  to  scale;  the  top 
and  bottom  of  the  figure  are  dorsal  and  ventral,  respectively;  anterior  is 
to  the  left  and  posterior  to  the  right). 


tic  acid  (EDTA),  pH  7.8  (Silverman  et  al..  1983,  1987). 
Alternatively,  glutaraldehyde  was  added  directly  to  the 
gills  in  freshwater  mussel  Ringer's  solution  with  or  without 
EDTA.  Gills  were  exposed  to  the  fixative  for  2  h.  During 
fixation,  the  gills  were  cut  dorso-ventrally  (parallel  to  gill 
filaments)  into  strips  of  5-8  mm.  Following  fixation,  gill 
strips  were  washed  three  times  for  5  min  each  in  either 
30  mM  tris-HCl  or  phosphate  buffer,  pH  7.8,  and  post- 
fixed  in  1%  aqueous  osmium  tetroxide  for  1  h.  After  os- 
mication,  the  gill  strips  were  washed  three  times  for  10 
min  each  in  deionized  water  and  then  dehydrated  in  a 
graded  ethanol  series  (10  min  in  50%,  70%,  80%,  90%, 
95%,  and  3X10  min  in  100%).  Two  resins,  Spurr's  low 
viscosity  (Poly sciences.  Inc.)  and  LR  White  hard  grade 
(EMS,  Inc.),  were  used.  Gill  strips  to  be  embedded  in  LR 
White  were  placed  in  a  1:1  resin/ethanol  mixture  for  20 
min  and  then  into  100%  LR  White  resin  for  24  h.  Fol- 


OSTIA-ASSOCIATED  MUSCULATURE 


455 


lowing  the  overnight  incubation,  fresh  LR  White  resin 
was  added,  and  the  gill  strips  were  embedded  flat  in  alu- 
minum pans  at  60°C  for  48  h.  Gill  strips  embedded  in 
Spurr's  resin  were  initially  placed  in  100%  propylene  ox- 
ide, 3  X  20  min,  followed  by  graded  propylene  oxide/ 
resin  series  (20  min  at  1 : 1 ,  1 :2,  1 :3,  1 :4,  3  X  1  h  at  100% 
resin,  and  100%.  for  24  h)  and  final  embedding  in  fresh 
resin  at  60°C  for  48  h. 

Gills  were  sectioned  for  light  microscopy  with  a  Reich- 
ert-Jung  Ultracut  E  ultramicrotome  at  0.5-2.0  ^m  thick- 
ness with  glass  knives  and  for  transmission  electron  mi- 
croscopy (TEM)  at  60-90  nm  thickness  with  a  diamond 
knife.  Sections  for  light  microscopy  were  stained  according 
to  a  tribasic  staining  procedure  developed  by  Grimley 
( 1 964 ).  The  gill  was  sectioned  in  two  planes:  ( 1 )  anterior- 
posterior  cross-sections,  and  (2)  frontal  sections  (en  face) 
across  the  surface  of  the  filaments.  Sections  for  light  mi- 
croscopy were  examined  and  photographed  with  a  Nikon 
Microphot-FXA  microscope.  Sections  for  TEM  were 
stained  with  3%  uranyl  acetate  for  2  min  followed  by 
Reynolds'  (1963)  lead  citrate  for  2-5  min.  The  sections 
were  examined  with  a  JOEL  100  CX  transmission  electron 
microscope  operating  at  80  kV. 

The  light  micrographs  of  isolated  chitinous  rods  were 
prepared  by  cutting  gills  into  5-6  mm  longitudinal  strips 
along  the  filaments  and  incubating  the  strips  in  calcium- 
free  Ringer's  solution  containing  1000  U/ml  collagenase 
IV  (Sigma,  St.  Louis).  After  12  h,  the  rods  were  collected 
by  repeated  centrifugations  of  the  collagenase  treated  gill 
at  50  X  g  for  5  min  and  viewed  with  an  Nikon  Diaphot 
inverted  microscope. 

Neurotransmitter  application 

Gills  were  excised  and  placed  in  individual  polystyrene 
petri  dishes  containing  freshwater  mussel  Ringer's,  pH 
7.8.  Gills  were  cut  in  half  dorso-ventrally,  and  incubated 
in  the  Ringer's  solution  for  30  min  with  changes  every  10 
min.  The  diameter  of  the  ostia  and  the  internal  ostia,  and 
gill  movement,  were  monitored  with  an  inverted  Nikon 
Diaphot  microscope  for  5  min  before  fixing.  One  of  the 
gill  halves  was  fixed  without  being  exposed  to  putative 
transmitter  substances  by  the  addition  of  an  equal  volume 
of  fixative  (4%  formalin  and  4%  glutaraldehyde  in  mussel 
Ringer's,  pH  7.8)  directly  onto  the  tissue.  Before  fixing 
the  control,  we  placed  the  other  half  of  the  bisected  gill 
in  a  mussel  Ringer's  solution  containing  10~5  M  neuro- 
transmitter  [acetylcholine,  dopamine,  gamma  aminobu- 
tyric  acid  (GABA),  epinephrine,  norepinephrine,  or  se- 
rotonin], pH  7.8. 

Preparation  of  gills  for  scanning  electron  microscopy 

After  fixation,  the  gills  were  washed  in  a  30  mM  tris- 
HC1  or  phosphate  buffer,  pH  7.8,  for  1 5  min  with  changes 


every  5  min,  and  then  osmicated  in  1%.  aqueous  osmium 
tetroxide  for  1  h.  After  osmication,  gills  were  washed  in 
deionized  water  for  15  min  with  changes  every  5  min. 
Fine  micro-dissection  tools  were  used  to  separate  the  op- 
posing gill  lamellae  by  severing  the  interlamellar  septa 
and  exposing  the  central  water  channel  epithelium  (see 
Fig.  1).  Gills  were  cut  dorsal-ventrally  (parallel  to  fila- 
ments) every  6-10  mm  and  dehydrated  in  the  graded 
ethanol  series.  The  tissues  were  then  stacked  perpendicular 
to  one  another  and  wrapped  in  lens  paper  to  ensure  that 
they  would  remain  flat  during  critical  point  drying.  Gills 
were  critical  point  dried  (Denton  Vacuum,  Inc.)  and 
mounted  on  stubs.  The  water  channel  epithelium  was  ori- 
ented as  the  facing  surface.  Specimens  were  sputter  coated 
with  gold/palladium  (20  nm)  and  viewed  with  a  Hitachi 
S-500  scanning  electron  microscope  (SEM)  with  a  working 
distance  of  30  mm,  operated  at  25  kV. 

Neurotransmitter  effect  on  the  canal  ostial  diameter 

The  dimensions  of  the  internal  ostia  of  gills  exposed  to 
transmitter  substance  were  obtained  from  scanning  elec- 
tron micrographs  and  assessed  quantitatively.  Samples  for 
scanning  electron  microscopy  were  selected  at  random 
from  control  and  treated  tissues  that  had  remained  flat 
after  critical  point  drying.  Three  or  more  tissues  were  se- 
lected from  each  treatment  group,  and  three  low  magni- 
fication micrographs  of  each  sample  were  taken  from  the 
first  three  separate  fields  of  the  water  channel  region 
brought  into  view.  Image-analysis  was  performed  on  the 
resulting  SEM  negatives.  Ostial  surface  area  and  other 
average  ostial  dimensions  (i.e.,  perimeter  and  diameter) 
were  calculated  from  digitized  images  using  densitometry 
and  stereology  software  (Image- 1 /AT  IM5000).  Statistics 
are  based  on  paired  Student's  t-tests  with  significance  set 
at  P  <  0.05. 

Assay  of  gill  ciliary  activity 

While  the  gross  muscular  responses  in  the  gill  to  various 
neurotransmitters  was  being  monitored,  ciliary  activity 
was  observed  to  increase  when  the  gills  were  exposed  to 
serotonin  or  dopamine.  The  changes  in  lateral  ciliary  ac- 
tivity in  Ligumia  gills  were  assayed  by  the  procedures  of 
Paparo  (1980).  Ciliary  beats  per  second  was  determined 
by  synchronizing  the  activity  with  the  rate  of  flashing  of 
a  calibrated  strobe  light.  Gills  were  placed  in  dishes  de- 
signed to  allow  pondwater  to  flow  through  at  a  rate  of  0.5 
ml/min.  Initial  measurements  of  the  rate  of  ciliary  beating 
in  pondwater  over  60  min  were  used  as  control  values. 
The  effect  of  serotonin  or  dopamine  on  the  ciliary  activity 
was  analyzed  after  the  petri  dish  contents  were  replaced 
with  fresh  pondwater  containing  either  serotonin  or  do- 
pamine and  then  continuing  the  flow  at  0.5  ml/min  with 


~^r  x. 
*  ^?     ^ 


~  • '  •  :' 


Figure  2.  Light  micrograph  of  a  cross-section  of  the  gill  ofAnodonta  (anterior/posterior  to  the  left/right). 
Underlying  the  filaments  (F)  is  the  muscular  band  (NM)  that  traverses  in  an  anterior-posterior  direction. 
The  muscles  insert  (I)  onto  the  calcified  chitinous  rods  (R)  that  support  the  filaments.  The  insertion  occurs 
as  fibers  of  muscle  extend  from  the  main  band  and  contacts  each  rod.  The  rods  are  surrounded  by  a  mu- 


OSTIA-ASSOCIATED  MUSCULATURE 


457 


fresh  pondwater  containing  the  appropriate  neurotrans- 
mitter.  This  application  of  the  neurotransmitter  solution 
was  maintained  for  1  h;  then  the  pondwater  was  removed 
and  replaced  by  fresh  pondwater  lacking  neurotransmitter, 
and  the  gill  was  monitored  for  another  hour.  The  con- 
centrations of  serotonin  and  dopamine  tested  ranged  from 
10  6to  1(T4  M. 

Rhodamine-1 23  treatment  of  gill  explants 

Mitochondria!  activity  in  epithelial  cells  of  the  water 
canal  was  demonstrated  with  a  mitochondrial  fluorescence 
stain,  rhodamine-123  (Johnson  el  ai,  1980),  a  positively 
charged  lipophilic  molecule  that  interacts  specifically  with 
mitochondrial  membranes.  Isolated  gills  were  examined 
with  a  confocal  imaging  system  supplied  by  Bio-Rad  Lab- 
oratories (Richmond.  California).  Gills  were  excised  from 
animals,  and  the  interlamellar  septa  were  cut  to  expose 
the  central  water  channel.  The  gills  were  incubated  for  20 
min  in  pondwater  containing  10  Mg/ml  rhodamine-123. 
Following  incubation,  the  gills  were  placed  on  glass  slides 
and  covered  with  coverslips  with  petroleum  jelly  at  the 
corners.  The  samples  were  viewed  under  a  Nikon  Micro- 
phot-FXA  microscope  equipped  for  confocal  imaging. 
Serial  images  along  the  length  of  the  water  canals  were 
captured  by  digitizing  on  the  image  enhancement  com- 
puter. 

Results 

Canal-associated  musculature 

Two  bands  of  musculature  are  directly  associated  with 
the  water  canals  of  the  gill.  Located  at  the  base  of  the 
filaments  are  relatively  thick  bands  of  musculature,  70- 
72  ^m  and  20-23  ^m  in  diameter  for  Anodonta  and  Lig- 
umia,  respectively.  These  muscle  bands  underlie  the  fil- 


aments and  are  oriented  in  the  anterior-posterior  direction 
along  the  entire  length  of  the  gill  (Fig.  2).  The  muscle 
bands  occur  periodically,  alternating  with  rows  of  ostia 
located  at  the  base  of  the  filaments  (Fig.  3).  Muscle  bands 
occur  approximately  every  335  ^m  and  every  180  ^m  in 
Anodonta  and  Ligumia,  respectively.  The  rows  of  ostial 
openings  connect  the  mantle  cavity  to  the  water  canal  at 
the  base  of  the  filaments  (Figs.  1-3).  The  muscle  bands 
lie  perpendicular  to,  and  run  between,  septations  in  the 
parallel  calcified  chitinous  rods  that  support  the  filaments 
(Figs.  3,  4).  The  muscle  appears  to  be  attached  to  the  end 
of  the  rods  (Fig.  4).  Indentations  are  observed  at  the  sep- 
aration points  along  the  individual  discontinuous  chitin- 
ous rods.  Contraction  of  the  muscle  bands  pulls  the  rods 
of  adjacent  filaments  together,  reducing  the  water  canal 
opening  to  a  slit  oriented  in  the  dorsal-ventral  direction. 
There  is  little  musculature  located  in  the  underlying  con- 
nective tissue  in  the  vicinity  of  the  water  canal  (Fig.  2) 
except  where  the  water  canal  approaches  the  basal  surface 
of  the  central  water  channel  epithelium. 

Another  distinct  band  of  musculature  is  located  at  the 
base  of  the  canal  near  the  opening  of  the  water  canal  into 
the  water  channel  (Fig.  5).  These  muscular  bands  are  ap- 
proximately 2 1  /im  in  thickness  in  Ligumia  and  are  ori- 
ented dorso-ventrally  (Fig.  6).  The  muscle  bands  are  on 
either  side  of  rows  of  canals  and  send  muscle  fibers  into 
the  base  of  the  canal  epithelium,  that  forms  the  IO  (Figs. 
6-9).  This  musculature  appears  to  have  the  ability  to  con- 
trol the  diameter  of  the  IO  (Fig.  9). 

Neurotransmitter  effects  on  canal-associated 
musculature 

The  two  muscular  systems  described  above  responded 
similarly  to  the  exogenous  putative  neurotransmitters  to 
which  the  gills  were  exposed.  The  addition  of  serotonin 
to  the  gills  //;  vitro  resulted  in  an  immediate  relaxation  of 


copolysaccharide  substance  that  appears  darkly  stained  in  this  micrograph  (arrowheads).  Ostia  are  not  visible 
in  this  micrograph  as  they  alternate  with  muscular  bands  along  the  base  of  the  filaments  (see  Fig.  3).  Bar 
=  50  Mm. 

Figure  3.  Light  micrograph  of  a  gill  from  Anodonta  cut  across  the  face  of  the  filaments  in  the  dorso- 
ventral  plane.  This  section  is  below  the  base  of  the  filaments  and  demonstrates  the  alternation  of  water  canal 
ostia  (O)  with  nerve-muscle  bands  (NM).  The  periodicity  of  this  alternation  of  structures  is  readily  apparent. 
Note  that  the  section  is  cut  through  the  calcified  rods  (R).  The  rods  taper  (arrowheads)  and  become  discon- 
tinuous every  335  Mm.  an<3  it 's  at  tms  tapered  site  that  the  muscle  bands  interact  with  the  rods.  Mucopoly- 
saccharide  (Silverman  ct  al.  1983)  material  (P)  associated  with  the  rods  is  evident  at  the  tapered  sites.  A 
few  calcium  concretions  (CC)  are  seen  in  the  connective  tissue  of  this  micrograph.  Bar  =  100  ^m. 

Figure  4.  Whole  mounts  of  calcified  chitinous  rods  from  gills  digested  with  collagenase.  (a)  Is  a  portion 
of  a  rod  in  which  soft  tissue  has  been  completely  digested  away.  Rods  are  discontinuous  allowing  the  nerve 
muscle  tract  to  pass  in  the  anterior-posterior  direction  between  adjacent  filaments.  The  ends  of  the  rods 
where  muscles  attach  are  flared  and  indented  (arrowhead),  (b)  Is  partially  digested  with  collagenase  and  the 
remains  of  soft  tissue/mucopolysaccharide  (arrowhead)  can  be  seen  inserting  on  two  rods  (R)  oriented  end 
to  end.  The  internal  darker  portion  of  the  rod  is  calcified.  The  less  dense  perimeter  contains  layers  of  less 
calcified  mucopolysaccharide.  Bars  in  a  and  b  =  2  tim. 


f 

% 


WC 


,8'; 


; 

«-     * 

**. 

\. 

»     \ 

.A 

» 

* 

•         -                                      V 

A                » 

1 

*••••  • 

/           '' 

ME 


WC 


Figure  5.  A  TEM  of  the  base  of  the  water  channel  of  a  Ligumia  gill.  The  water  channel  (WC)  is  located 
at  the  bottom  of  the  micrograph.  The  epithelium  of  a  water  canal  near  its  junction  with  the  water  channel 
is  located  at  the  top  of  the  micrograph.  Note  the  major  obliquely  striated  muscle  band  (IM)  located  in  the 
connective  tissue  at  the  base  of  the  water  channel  epithelium.  Mitochondria  (m)  are  indicated  in  the  mi- 


OSTIA-ASSOCIATED  MUSCULATURE 


459 


the  external  and  internal  musculature,  thereby  increasing 
the  diameter  of  the  ostia  (Fig.  10)  and  the  internal  ostia 
(Fig.  1 1).  These  results  were  visible  by  gross  observation 
of  the  preparations  with  an  inverted  light  microscope  (Fig. 
10).  The  relaxation  was  maintained  throughout  the  5- 
min  observation  period.  In  contrast,  none  of  the  other 
transmitters  tested  appeared  to  have  any  visible  effect. 

The  internal  ostia  were  examined  by  SEM  and  the  av- 
erage dimensions  of  the  internal  ostia  were  measured  in 
control  and  experimental  treatment  groups  (Table  I).  The 
average  dimensions  of  the  internal  ostia  in  a  serotonin- 
treated  gill  are  2-3  fold  larger  than  controls  (Table  I,  Fig. 
1 1 ).  The  internal  ostia  of  the  controls  have  a  distinct  long 
axis  (height)  showing  indented  edges  (Figs.  11.  12).  The 
fully  relaxed  serotonin-treated  ostia  have  a  smooth,  uni- 
form oval  to  circular  shape  (Figs.  11.  13).  The  addition 
of  acetylcholine.  dopamine,  GABA,  epinephrine,  or  nor- 
epinephrine  caused  no  observable  changes  in  the  size  of 
the  internal  ostia,  implying  that  the  contractile  state  of 
the  gill  musculature  was  not  affected  by  these  agents  (Ta- 
ble I). 

Rhodamine-123  treatment 

The  muscular  control  of  canal  ostia.  and  the  regulation 
of  water  flow  through  the  canal,  are  consistent  with  our 
hypothesis  that  the  water  canal  epithelial  cells  are  a  major 
site  of  ion  transport  in  the  gill.  When  gills  were  incubated 
in  rhodamine-123,  fluorescence  was  specifically  localized 
with  the  mitochondria-rich  cells  of  the  canal  epithelium. 
When  the  intact  living  gill  was  visualized  with  confocal 
optics,  the  cells  lining  the  water  canals  displayed  the 
greatest  fluorescence  because  of  their  high  mitochondrial 
content  (Fig.  14).  Optical  sectioning  shows  that  this  high 
activity  is  in  every  water  canal  epithelial  cell  and  extends 
along  the  entire  length  of  the  water  canal. 


Gill  ciliary  activity 

In  vitro,  gills  incubated  in  pondwater  showed  a  consis- 
tent rate  of  ciliary  activity  ( 1 5  beats/s)  during  the  1-h  con- 
trol observation  (Fig.  15).  Addition  of  10~4  M  serotonin 
caused  an  immediate  increase  in  ciliary  activity  that 
peaked  within  20  min  at  24  beats/s.  The  high  ciliary  rate 
was  maintained  until  serotonin  was  removed  40  min  later, 
and  the  ciliary  beat  returned  to  base  line  about  40  min 
thereafter. 

Dopamine  had  an  effect  on  ciliary  activity,  but  it  dif- 
fered from  that  of  serotonin  (Fig.  15).  The  increased  ciliary 
activity  peaked  at  20  beats/s  upon  the  addition  of  10  4 
M  dopamine,  but  40  min  were  needed  to  reach  the  peak 
ciliary  rate.  After  dopamine  was  removed,  the  ciliary  ac- 
tivity immediately  dropped  to  baseline.  The  response  to 
both  dopamine  and  serotonin  was  dose-dependent  as 
shown  in  Figure  16. 

Discussion 

Most  studies  of  water  flow  through  eulamellibranch  gills 
stress  the  role  of  ciliary  activity  as  the  driving  force  for 
water  flow  (Jorgenson.  1982;  Silvester  1988;  Sleigh,  1989). 
Indeed,  many  flow  measurements  and  coupled  mathe- 
matical analyses  indicate  that  ciliary  activity  is  sufficient 
to  account  for  the  water  flow  (Jorgenson,  1989;  Sleigh, 
1989).  These  models  treat  water  canals  as  hollow  tubes 
of  fixed  dimensions  for  the  calculations.  While  such  mod- 
els are  useful  for  studying  water  flow  through  gill  systems, 
they  are  constrained  by  the  underlying  assumptions.  The 
measured  2-3  fold  difference  in  the  internal  ostia  dimen- 
sions between  control  and  serotonin-treated  gills  indicates 
that  effective  water  canal  size  and  its  regulation  are  po- 
tentially important  factors  to  be  considered  for  water  flow 
through  the  gill  of  unionids.  The  substantial  increase  in 
ostial  size  coupled  to  the  increase  in  ciliary  activity  known 


tochondria-rich  water  canal  epithelial  cells.  Bar  =  1  urn. 

Figure  6.  A  light  micrograph  of  a  face  section  through  the  gill  of  Ligumia.  The  section  is  cut  through 
the  gill  just  above  the  base  of  the  water  channel  epithelium.  Located  between  and  associated  with  the  canals 
(C)  are  bands  of  muscle  (IM)  traversing  in  a  dorsal/ventral  direction.  These  muscle  bands  alternate  with 
rows  of  canals  and  send  fibers  to  the  base  of  the  canal  epithelial  cells.  Muscle  bands  are  not  seen  in  every 
location  between  water  canals  as  the  section  is  at  a  slightly  oblique  plane.  Bar  =  60  ^m. 

Figure  7.  Low  magnification  TEM  of  Ligumia  gill  indicating  that  the  major  internal  muscle  band  (IM) 
lying  at  the  base  of  the  water  channel  (WC)  epithelium  branches  and  has  numerous  muscular  extensions 
(ME).  These  extensions  eventually  end  in  the  region  of  the  internal  ostia  with  several  muscle  fibers  inserting 
at  the  base  of  the  water  canal  (C)  epithelium  (see  Fig.  8).  Most  of  the  cytoplasm  of  the  two  epithelia  observed 
is  occupied  by  glycogen  (g)  and  mitochondria  (m).  Bar  =  2  ^m. 

Figure  8.  Higher  magnification  TEM  of  Anodonia  gill  showing  that  the  muscle  extensions  end  in  thin, 
finger-like  processes  consisting  of  only  a  few  muscle  fibers.  These  fibers  are  obliquely  striated  fibers,  and  the 
inset  indicates  the  presence  of  thick  and  thin  filaments.  The  muscle  is  inserting  in  the  basal  region  of  the 
water  canal  near  the  water  channel  epithelium  (E)  and  has  hemidesmosome-like  electron-dense  material  at 
the  muscle-connective  tissue  interface  (inset).  Note  that  in  this  region  of  the  water  channel  epithelial  cells 
are  ciliated  (arrowhead).  Bar  =  1  ^m;  inset  bar  =  0.25  ^m. 


460 


D.  B.  GARDINER  ET  AL. 


- 


Figure  9.  A  light  micrograph  from  Anoclonla  gill  showing  the  finger-like  muscle  extensions  (ME)  on 
either  side  of  a  water  canal  (C)  at  the  base  of  the  epithelial  cells  where  the  canal  enters  the  water  channel 
(WC)  at  the  internal  ostia  (IO).  Bar  =  60  ^m. 


OSTIA-ASSOCIATED  MUSCULATURE 


461 


to  occur  with  serotonin  in  some  bivalves  (Gosselin  ct  a/.. 
1962;  Aiello  and  Guideri,  1966;Aiello,  1970:  Paparo  and 
Murphy,  1975;  Jorgenson,  1976;  Capatane  et  at..  1978; 
Sanderson  and  Satir,  1982;  Sanderson  et  al..  1985;  this 
study)  makes  canal  size  regulation  an  important,  under- 
estimated contributor  to  water  flow  regulation.  The  three- 
fold difference  in  ostia  dimensions  with  serotonin  treat- 
ment may  be  an  over-estimate  of  the  normal  conditions 
based  on  the  potential  for  some  partial  contraction  of  the 
muscle.  We  developed  our  methods  to  minimize  fixation 
artifact,  but  the  microscopic  preparations  would  likely 
lead  to  reduced  ostial  dimensions  (shrinkage)  rather  than 
enlargement.  Our  data  demonstrate  the  potential  range 
within  which  the  mussels  can  regulate  canal  openings  and 
water  flow  with  the  ostial  musculature. 

The  general  orientation  of  the  musculature  associated 
with  the  ostia  and  internal  ostia  was  the  same  in  the  two 
unionid  genera  we  examined  and  is  thus  likely  to  be  the 
generalized  pattern  for  the  unionids.  These  muscles  are 
obliquely  striated  and  have  not  been  well-characterized 
(Ridewood,  1903;  Ortmann,  1911;  Kays  et  al,  1990; 
Richard  et  al.,  1991).  Their  organization  and  their  asso- 
ciation with  the  ostia  suggest  that  the  axes  for  movement 
and  for  regulation  of  the  two  openings  are  different.  The 
ostia  are  regulated  by  the  muscles.  When  they  contract, 
adjacent  chitinous  rods  of  adjacent  filaments  are  pulled 
toward  one  another  closing  the  ostia.  During  relaxation, 
the  tension  on  the  rods  is  released,  allowing  the  filaments 
to  separate  and  the  ostia  located  at  the  base  of  the  filaments 
to  open.  Such  a  mechanism  suggests  that  the  gill  as  a 
whole  would  have  a  "postural  tone"  under  normal  con- 
ditions. This  can  be  confirmed  by  watching  the  accordion- 
like  movements  of  the  gill  due  to  spontaneous  contrac- 
tions, and  the  expansion  of  the  gill  when  relaxed  following 
the  addition  of  serotonin.  The  muscle  bands  at  the  IO  are 
perpendicular  to  the  muscle  bands  at  the  ostia.  They  are 
oriented  dorso-ventrally  along  the  gill  axis  and  in  close 


Table  I 

Average  internal  inlia  .v/rt-  in  Ligumia  subrostrata  gills  following 
exogenous  treatment  with  biogenic  amines 


Height 


Width 


Perimeter 


ACh 

39.7 

±8.0 

12.1 

t 

3.2 

93.9 

±21.2 

ACh-control 

33.1 

±9.1 

12.3 

i 

2.9 

82.2 

±  22.1 

Dopamme 

24.3 

±0.9 

12.3 

t 

1.5 

65.9 

±    3.3 

Dopamine-control 

30.6 

±0.9 

10.3 

± 

0.5 

75.6 

±     1.9 

Epi 

34.5 

±  5.3 

11.3 

t 

1.4 

81.9 

±  11.9 

Epi-control 

27.4 

±  5.3 

10.2 

t 

1.4 

67.2 

±  11.5 

Norepi 

^9  9 

±4.6 

9.4 

± 

1.3 

71.4 

±  11.2 

Norepi-control 

31.9 

±  1.9 

11.3 

i 

2.9 

74.9 

±    7.5 

GABA 

22.5 

±  1.8 

15.9 

± 

1.9 

64.6 

±    6.3 

GABA-control 

23.8 

±  4.5 

13.2 

t 

1.2 

60.4 

±    9.0 

Serotonin 

57.9 

±  2.7* 

24.4 

± 

1.1* 

147.3 

±    6.9* 

Serotonin-control 

33.1 

±  3.8 

10.0 

1 

0.9 

78.3 

±    8.2 

All  measurements  are  in  microns.  Height  and  width  refer  to  the  longest 
and  shortest  axes  of  the  oval  shaped  ostia,  respectively.  Data  are  means 
±  standard  error  (n  >  3),  ACh  =  Acetylcholine,  Epi  =  Epinephrine, 
GABA  =  Gamma  Aminobutyric  Acid.  Norepi  =  Norepinephrine. 

*  Significantly  different  from  controls,  P  <  0.01. 


proximity  to  the  IO;  they  exert  control  by  sending  a  few 
muscle  fibers  to  the  base  of  the  epithelial  cells  surrounding 
the  IO.  When  these  muscle  bands  contract,  the  inserting 
fibers  pull  on  the  IO,  creating  an  elongated  shape,  and 
causing  an  indented  appearance  on  the  edges  of  the  IO. 
Increased  muscular  contraction  elongates  and  closes  the 
opening.  The  SEM,  TEM,  and  bright  field  images,  com- 
paring control  to  serotonin-treated  gills,  are  all  consistent 
with  this  proposed  mechanism  of  action. 

The  results  reported  here  indirectly  suggest  that  sero- 
tonin is  a  relaxing  agent  for  the  muscle  bands  we  have 
described.  While  the  results  have  not  been  confirmed 
electrically,  they  are  consistent  with  such  experiments  in 
other  molluscan  systems  (Cambridge,  1959;  Twarog  and 
Cole,  1972;  Satchell  and  Twarog,  1978;  Kobayashi  and 


Figure  10.  Comparison  of  whole  mount  light  micrographs  of  control  (a)  and  serotonin-treated  (b)  Ano- 
i/onla  gills.  The  micrographs  show  the  surface  of  the  filaments,  allowing  observation  of  the  ostia  (arrowheads). 
In  (a),  the  control  gill  ostia  are  barely  discernable  as  the  lighter  areas  because  the  adjacent  filaments  (F)  are 
pulled  toward  one  another  closing  the  space  between  filaments.  Gills  in  this  condition  have  few  ostial  openings. 
In  contrast,  a  gill  treated  with  IO"5  M  serotonin  (b)  shows  filaments  that  are  farther  from  one  another 
allowing  ostial  openings  to  enlarge.  Bars  in  a  and  b  =  50  ^m. 

Figure  11.  Scanning  electron  micrographs  of  the  water  channel  epithelium  (WCE)  of  Ligumia  showing 
the  internal  ostia  (IO)  as  they  enter  the  water  channel,  (a)  Is  an  untreated  control  gill.  Note  both  the  size 
and  shape  of  the  IO  openings.  The  long  axis  (height)  has  a  dorsal/ventral  orientation.  Their  edges,  particularly 
those  on  the  dorsal  and  ventral  ends,  tend  to  have  an  indented  appearance,  (b)  Is  a  gill  that  has  been  treated 
with  IO"5  M  serotonin.  Relaxation  of  musculature  allows  the  IO  to  fully  expand.  The  ostia  still  have  a  dorsal- 
ventral  orientation  although  not  nearly  as  pronounced.  The  ostia  have  an  oval  to  round  shape  and  the 
indentations  seen  in  (a)  are  absent.  The  oval  orientation  is  likely  due  to  the  orientation  of  the  underlying 
musculature,  (c)  Is  a  gill  that  has  been  treated  with  pH  5  buffer  to  stimulate  full  contraction.  Note  the 
exaggerated  dorsal-ventral  orientation  and  deep  indentations  of  the  ostial  edges  oriented  in  the  same  direction. 
The  "pull"  by  the  underlying  musculature  has  occluded  the  IO  opening.  Bars  in  a,  b  and  c  =  10  nm. 


nw 
-• 


Figure  12.  High  magnification  SEM  micrographs  of  Ligwnia  gills  viewing  the  water  channel  epithelium 
(WCE).  (a)  Is  from  a  control  gill  showing  the  elongation  in  the  dorsal-ventral  direction.  This  internal  ostium 
is  almost  completely  occluded.  The  indentations  of  the  IO  border  are  evident  (arrows)  as  well,  (b)  Is  an 
internal  ostium  from  a  serotonin-treated  ( 10  '  .I/)  gill.  The  dorsal-ventral  longitudinal  orientation  is  evident. 


OSTIA-ASSOCIATED  MUSCULATURE 


463 


Hasimoto.  1982).  Acetylcholine,  dopamine,  norepineph- 
rine,  epinephrine,  and  GABA  were  neither  excitatory  nor 
inhibitory  in  our  bioassay.  These  results  clearly  do  not 
exclude  any  of  these  substances  as  putative  excitatory 
transmitters  because  bath  application  may  not  allow  these 
agents  to  reach  their  targets.  We  were  able  to  demonstrate 
a  dose-response  relationship  of  ciliary  beat  for  both  se- 
rotonin and  dopamine,  but  our  bioassay  was  not  suffi- 
ciently sensitive,  and  so  no  dose-response  relationship  for 
serotonin-induced  muscular  relaxation  was  demonstrated 
only  an  all-or-none  relaxation  response  occurred. 

Although  these  muscles  have  previously  been  ignored, 
their  importance  to  the  functions  of  the  unionid  gill  should 
not  be  overlooked.  Evidence  demonstrating  that  the  gill 
is  the  predominant  site  of  ion  regulation  in  unionid  mus- 
sels is  convincing  (Dietz  and  Findley,  1980;  Dietz  and 
Graves,  1981;  Dietz  and  Hagar.  1990).  Further,  more  ev- 
idence is  accumulating  (Kays  el  ai.  1990;  this  study)  that 
the  epithelial  cells  of  the  water  canals  are  important  os- 
moregulatory  cells.  The  high  mitochondria!  content  and 
activity  (as  demonstrated  here  by  rhodamine-123  exper- 
iments), surface  area  calculations,  considerable  basal  and 
lateral  membrane  infolding,  and  the  high  levels  of  cyto- 
chrome  oxidase  activity  (Kays  el  ai,  1990)  shown  by  these 
cells  all  suggest  osmoregulatory  function.  Coordinated 
muscle  and  ciliary  activity  may  allow  finer  control  and  a 
wider  range  of  regulation,  including  a  shut-down  of  water 
flow.  The  coordinated  control  of  ciliary  and  muscular  ac- 
tivity is  apparent,  at  least  in  response  to  serotonin. 

No-flow  conditions  do  occur  in  some  unionid  species 
during  reproduction.  In  the  Lampsilinae,  the  central  water 
chambers  housing  embryos  are  physiologically  isolated 
from  the  water  flow  through  the  mantle  cavity  (Silverman 


o 

(D 


(A 


.> 
"o 

i 

o 

0  20  40  60  80  100  120  140  160  180 
Time  (min) 

Figure  15.  Lateral  ciliary  activity  of  a  representative  (of  5)  Ligumia 
gill  in  response  to  application  of  exogenous  serotonin  and  dopamine. 
Gills  were  exposed  either  to  10  *  M  serotonin  (open  squares)  or  IO"4  A/ 
dopamine  (open  circles)  for  1  h.  Initiation  of  the  treatment  is  indicated 
by  the  upward  pointing  arrow  and  termination  by  the  downward  pointing 
arrow. 


et  ai.  1987;  Richard  cl  ai,  1991).  We  speculate  that  the 
mechanism  for  reduced  water  flow  into  the  brood  chamber 
is,  in  part,  regulation  by  canal  ostial  musculature. 

Mathematical  treatments  of  the  hydrodynamics  of  wa- 
ter flow  through  mussel  gills  do  not  completely  fit  the 
available  data.  Silvester  (1988)  has  recently  concluded, 
after  an  elegant  treatment  ofMytilus  gill  ciliary  mechanics, 
that  faster  flow  than  can  be  accounted  for  by  known  ciliary 
activity  actually  occurs.  Indeed,  his  final  statement,  "one 
should  perhaps  be  alert  to  the  possibility  that  other  systems 
in  the  mussel  may  be  contributing  to  the  pumping  per- 
formance" (Silvester,  1988).  could  allude  to  the  possibility 


The  IO  opening  is  not  indented  as  seen  in  (a).  This  field  allows  a  clear  view  through  the  1O  and  into  the 
water  canal.  At  the  filament  side  of  the  water  canal  is  an  ostium  that  is  delimited  by  the  filaments  on  either 
side  of  the  ostium.  This  is  evident  by  the  dorsal-ventral,  straight  edges  (arrowheads)  of  the  ostium.  The 
muscle  bands  underlying  the  two  ostial  openings  lie  perpendicular  to  one  another  (not  shown),  but  both 
bands  work  to  close  their  respective  opening  in  the  dorsal-ventral  direction.  Bars  in  a  and  b  =  10  /jm 

Figure  13.  An  SEM  micrograph  of  Ligumia  gill  exposed  to  10~5  M  serotonin.  The  gill  has  been  prepared 
so  that  the  left  hand  side  of  the  micrograph  has  one  lamella  removed  to  expose  the  water  channel  epithelium 
(WC)  while  the  right  side  of  the  micrograph  contains  an  intact  lamella  and  filaments  (F).  Between  the 
filaments,  the  ostia  (O)  are  visible  and  fully  open,  displaying  the  dorsal-ventral  long-axis  orientation.  They 
clearly  demonstrate  the  limitations  on  their  size  being  set  by  the  inter-filament  distance.  The  space  between 
filaments  is  controlled  by  muscle  inserting  on  the  chitinous  rods  supporting  the  filaments.  The  internal  ostia 
(IO)  show  the  same  orientation.  Bar  =  50  ^m. 

Figure  14.  Face  view  of  Ligumia  gills  showing  the  mid-region  of  the  water  canals  (C)  passing  from  the 
gill  filaments  into  the  water  channel,  (a)  Is  a  control  light  micrograph  of  fixed  tissue  similar  to  that  seen  in 
Figure  6.  Note  the  size  of  the  water  canal  epithelial  cells  (arrow).  These  cells  have  previously  been  shown  to 
be  high  in  cytochrome  oxidase  activity  (Kays  et  ai.  1990).  (b)  Is  an  optical  section  through  a  living  gill 
accomplished  using  confocal  imaging  techniques.  The  gill  has  been  treated  with  rhodamine-123  to  highlight 
mitochondria!  location.  The  major  fluorescence  corresponds  to  the  cytoplasm  of  the  water  canal  epithelial 
cells  (arrows).  The  canal  cells  are  the  major  site  of  active  mitochondria.  There  is  some  auto-fluorescence 
associated  with  other  epithelia  of  the  gill,  but  it  is  minor  compared  with  that  seen  in  the  water  canal  epithelia 
cells.  Bars  in  a  and  b  =  50  urn. 


464 


D.  B.  GARDINER  ET  AL. 


16 


25 


S      20 


O 

as 


O 


15 


10 


CH  Control 
O5-HT 


0.001        0.01 


0.1 


Concentration  (M/L) 

Figure  16.  Dose-response  relationship  of  lateral  ciliary  beat  in  Lig- 
iimui  gills  following  the  application  of  serotonin  or  dopamine.  The  last 
three  ciliary  rate  measurements  of  the  initial  control  and  treatment  periods 
were  averaged  for  each  gill,  and  the  average  of  five  gills  are  presented 
(standard  error  <  0.5  beats/s). 


of  muscular  activity  aiding  ciliary  function.  Indeed,  the 
anterior-posterior  and  dorsal-ventral  contractions  of  the 
muscle  bands  in  the  gill  may  provide  additional  driving 
force  through  an  accordion-like  motion.  The  current  study 
indicates  that  these  muscles  are  likely  to  be  important 
contributors  to  water  flow  dynamics  across  the  molluscan 
gill,  at  least  for  the  unionids. 

Acknowledgments 

We  thank  Dr.  A.  Paparo  for  determination  of  ciliary 
activity,  Ms.  Beckey  Demler  of  the  LSU  Basic  Sciences 
Microscopy  Center  for  technical  help,  and  Ron  Bouchard 
for  photographic  assistance.  All  image  analysis  was  done 
in  the  Microscopy  Facility.  This  work  comprises  a  portion 
of  an  MS  thesis  (Louisiana  State  University)  by  DBG. 
This  work  was  supported  by  a  NSF  grant  DCB88-02320. 

Literature  Cited 

Aiello,  E.  1970.  Nervous  and  chemical  stimulation  of  gill  cilia  in  bivalve 
molluscs.  Phvxiol  /oil/.  43:  60-70. 

Aiello,  E.,  and  G.  Guidcri.  1966.  Relationship  between  5  hydroxytryp- 
tamine  and  nerve  stimulation  of  ciliary  activity.  ./.  Pharmacol.  Exp. 
Ther.  15-4:  517-523. 

Cambridge,  G.  W.,  J.  A.  Holgate,  and  J.  A.  Sharp.  1959.  A  pharma- 
cological analysis  of  the  contractile  mechanism  of  Mvlilus  muscle. 
./  Phyxiol.  148:451-464. 

Catapane,  E.  J.,  G.  B.  Stefano,  and  E.  Aiello.  1978.  Pharmacological 
study  of  the  reciprocal  dual  innervation  of  the  lateral  ciliated  gill 
epithelium  by  the  CNS  of  Mvlilux  edulis  (Bivalvia).  /  Exp.  Biol,  74: 
101-113. 


Dietz,  T.  H.,  and  A.  M.  Findley.  1980.     Ion-stimulated  ATPase  activity 

and  NaCl  uptake  in  the  gills  of  freshwater  mussels.  Can  J  Zool  58: 

917-923. 
Dietz,  T.  H..  and  S.  Y.  Graves.  1981.     Sodium  influx  in  isolated  gills 

of  the  freshwater  mussel  Ligumia  subroslrala.  J.  Coinp.  Phvsiol  143: 

185-190. 
Dietz,  T.  H.,  and  A.  F.  llagar.  1990.     Chloride  uptake  in  isolated  gills 

of  the  freshwater  mussel  Ligumia  subrostrata.  Can.  J  Zool.  68:  6- 

9. 
Galtsoff,  P.  S.  1964.     The  American  oyster  Crassoslrea  Virginia  Gmelin. 

U.  S  Fish  Wildlife  Sen:  Fish.  Bull.  64:  1-480. 
Gosselin,  R.  E.,  K.  E.  Moore,  and  A.  S.  Milton.  1962.     Physiological 

control  of  molluscan  gill  cilia  by  5-hydroxytryptamine.  J.  Gen.  Phvs- 

iol.  46:  277-296. 
Grimley,  P.  1964.     A  tribasic  stain  for  thin  sections  of  plastic-embedded, 

OsO4-fixed  tissue.  Slain  Tech.  39:  229-233. 
Johnson,  L.  V.,  M.  L.  Walsh,  and  L.  B.  Chen.  1980.     Localization  of 

mitochondria  in  living  cells  with  rhodamine-123.  Pmc.  Nail.  Acad 

Sci.  USA  77:  990-994. 
Jorgensen,  C.  B.  1976.     Comparative  studies  on  the  function  of  gills  in 

suspension  feeding  bivalves,  with  special  reference  to  effects  of  se- 
rotonin. Biol.  Bull.  151:  331-343. 
Jorgensen,  C.  B.  1982.     Fluid  mechanics  of  the  mussel  gill:  the  lateral 

cilia.  Mar.  Biol  70:  275-281. 
Jorgensen,  C.  B.  1989.     Water  processing  in  ciliary  feeders,  with  special 

reference  to  the  bivalve  filter  pump.  Coinp.  Bnicliem.  Physio/.  94A: 

383-394. 
Kays,  W.  T.,  H.  Silverman,  and  T.  H.  Dietz.  1990.     Water  channels 

and  water  canals  in  the  gill  of  the  freshwater  mussel,  Ligumia  sub- 
roslrala: ultrastructure  and  histochemistry.  J.  Exp.  Zool.  254:  256- 

269. 
Kobayashi,  M.,  and  T.  Hasimoto.  1982.     Antagonistic  responses  of  the 

radular  protractor  and  retractor  to  the  same  putative  neurotrans- 

mitters.  Comp.  Biochem.  Phvsiol.  72C:  343-348. 
Nelson,  T.  C.  1941.     On  the  locus  of  action  of  diantlin  upon  the  oyster's 

gills  as  revealed  by  the  effects  of  acetylcholine.  eserine,  and  adrenalin. 

Anal.  Rec  81:  88. 
Nelson,  T.  C.  and  J.  B.  Allison.  1940.     On  the  nature  and  action  of 

diantlin;  a  new  hormone-like  substance  carried  by  the  spermatozoa 

of  the  oyster.  ./.  Exp.  Zool.  85:  299-338. 
Ortmann,  A.  E.  1911.     A  monograph  of  the  Najades  of  Pennsylvania. 

Mem.  Carnegie  Mus.  4:  279-347. 
Paparo,  A.  1980.     The  regulation  of  intracellular  calcium  and  the  release 

of  neurotransmitters  in  the  mussel,  Mytilus  edulis.  Comp.  Biochem. 

Phyxiol.  66A:  517-520. 
Paparo,  A.  1988.     Ciliary  activity  on  the  ctenidium  of  bivalve  molluscs. 

Comp.  Biochem.  Phyxiol.  91C:  99-1  10. 
Paparo,  A.,  and  J.  A.  Murphy.  1975.     The  effect  of  Ca  on  the  rate  of 

beating  of  lateral  cilia  in  Myli/i/x  edit/is  I.  A  response  to  perfusion 

with  5-HT,  DA,  BOL,  and  PBZ.  Comp.  Biochem.  Phvsiol.  50C:  9- 

14. 

Reynolds,  E.  S.  1963.     The  use  of  lead  citrate  at  high  pH  as  an  electron- 
opaque  stain  in  electron  microscopy.  ./.  Cell.  Biol.  17:  208-213. 
Richard,  P.  K.,  T.  H.  Dielz,  and  H.  Silverman.  1991.     Structure  of  the 

gill  during  reproduction.  Anodonta  grandis,  Ligiimia  subrostrata  and 

Carunculina  pana  lexasensis.  Can.  J.  Zool.  (in  press). 
Rideuood,  \V.  G.  1903.     On  the  structure  of  the  gills  of  the  Lamelli- 

branchia.  Phil.  Trans.  Roy.  Soc.  London  195:  147-284. 
Riisgard,  II.  U.,  and  F.  Mohlenberg.  1979.     An  improved  automatic 

recording  apparatus  for  determining  the  filtration  rate  of  Mytilus 

edulis  as  a  function  of  size  and  algal  concentration.  Mar.  Biol.  52: 

61-67. 


OSTIA-ASSOCIATED  MUSCULATURE 


465 


Sanderson,  M.  J.,  E.  R.  Dirksen,  and  P.  Salir.  1985.  The  antagonistic 
effects  of  5-hydroxytryptamine  and  methylxanthine  on  the  gill  cilia 
ofMytilux  ediilis  Cell  Monl.  5:  293-309. 

Sanderson,  M.  J.,  and  P.  Satir.  1982.  Multiple  effects  of  ethanol  and 
5-hydroxytryptamine  on  the  gill  cilia  of  Mylihts  eilulix.  Cell  Molii 
2:  215-224. 

Satchel!,  D.  G.  and  B.  M.  Twarog.  1978.  Identification  of  5-hydroxy- 
tryptamme  (serotonin)  released  from  the  anterior  hyssus  retractor 
muscle  of  Mylilus  californianus  in  response  to  nerve  stimulation. 
Comp.  Biochem  P/miol.  59C:  81-85. 

Silverman,  II.  1989.  Form  and  function  of  calcium  concretions  in 
unionids.  Pp.  367-384  in  Origin.  Evolution,  and  Modern  .-l.v/uv/.v  <>l 
Bionwieralization  in  Plants  and  Animals.  Prmeedini-s  <>/  the  Fifth 
International  Symposium  on  Biomineralization,  R.  Crick,  ed.  Plenum 
Press,  New  York. 


Silverman,  H.,  W.  T.  Kays,  and  T.  H.  Dietz.  1987.  Maternal  calcium 
contribution  to  glochidial  shells  in  freshwater  mussels  (Eulamelh- 
branchia:  Unionidae).  ./  K\p.  /<«>/.  242:  137-146. 

Silverman,  H.,  \V.  L.  Steffens,  and  I.  11.  Dietz.  1983.  Calcium  con- 
cretions in  the  gills  of  a  freshwater  mussel  serve  as  a  calcium  reservoir 
during  periods  of  hypoxia.  /  K.\p.  Zool  227:  177-189. 

Silvester,  N.  1988.  Hydrodynamics  of  flow  in  Mytilus  gill.  J.  Exp. 
Mar  Biol.  Ecol.  120:  171-182. 

Sleigh,  M.  1989.  Adaptations  of  ciliary  systems  for  the  propulsion  of 
water  and  mucus.  C'nm/>  Biin'lwin  Mysiol.  94A:  359-364. 

Twarog,  B.  M.  1954.  Responses  of  a  molluscan  smooth  muscle  to  ace- 
tylcholine  and  5-hydroxytryptamine. ./,  Cell.  Comp.  Physio/.  44:  141- 
163. 

Twarog,  B.  M.,  and  R.  A.  Cole.  1972.  Relaxation  of  catch  in  a  molluscan 
smooth  muscle  II.  Effects  of  serotonin,  dopamine  and  related  com- 
pounds. Comp.  Biochem.  Physio/.  43A:  331-335. 


Reference:  #;W  Hull.  180:  466-474.  (June,  1941) 


On  the  Nature  of  Paddle  Cilia  and  Discocilia 


GRAHAM  SHORT*  AND  SIDNEY  L.  TAMM** 

Boston  University  Marine  Program,  Marine  Biological  Laboratory, 
Woods  Hole,  Massachusetts  02543 


Abstract.  Cilia  with  paddle-shaped  or  disc-shaped  tips 
enclosing  a  curved  end  of  the  axoneme  (paddle  cilia  or 
discocilia)  have  been  described  in  a  variety  of  marine  in- 
vertebrates. Although  numerous  studies,  in  which  fixed 
specimens  were  used,  claimed  that  paddle  cilia  and  dis- 
cocilia are  genuine  structures  of  unknown  function,  sev- 
eral studies,  in  which  fresh  living  material  was  used,  re- 
ported that  modified  cilia  are  artifacts.  We  have  re-inves- 
tigated a  recent  SEM  report  that  paddle  cilia  are  genuine 
organelles  in  veliger  larvae  of  marine  bivalves  (Campos 
and  Mann.  1988).  Using  high-speed  video  and  electronic 
flash  DIC  microscopy,  we  find  no  paddle  cilia  in  living 
larvae  of  Spisula  solidissima  and  Lymdits  pedicellatus. 
Hypotonic  seawater,  however,  induces  formation  of  pad- 
dle cilia  and  vesiculations  of  the  ciliary  membrane  in  these 
veligers,  as  does  the  hypotonic  SEM  fixative  used  by 
Campos  and  Mann  (1988).  Fixatives  that  are  isosmotic 
with  seawater,  on  the  other  hand,  do  not  induce  paddle 
cilia.  We  conclude  that  paddle  cilia  are  artifacts,  and  we 
propose  a  unifying  mechanism  to  explain  their  production 
in  various  animals  under  different  conditions. 

Introduction 

Cilia  with  a  distal  expansion  of  the  ciliary  membrane 
enclosing  a  looped  end  of  the  axoneme  (paddle  cilia  or 
discocilia)  have  been  described  in  a  variety  of  marine  in- 
vertebrates (Tamarin  el  al,  1974;  Oldfield,  1975;  Bergquist 
el  a/..  1977;  Dilly,  1977a,  b;  Ehlers  and  Ehlers,  1978; 
Heimler,  1978;  Storch  and  Alberti,  1978;  Arnold  and 
Williams-Arnold,  1980;  Bone  el  al,  1982;  Matera  and 
Davis,  1982;  Pfannenstiel,  1982;  Nielsen,  1987;  Campos 
and  Mann,  1988;  Durfot  el  al..  1990).  In  spite  of  older 
cytological  evidence  to  the  contrary  (Hartmann,  1953; 

Received  7  February  1991;  accepted  22  March  1991. 
*  Present  address:  Cell,  Molecular,  Neuroscience  Program,  University 
of  Hawaii,  Honolulu.  Hawaii  96822. 

*  To  whom  reprint  requests  should  be  sent. 


Lewin  and  Meinhart,  1953;  Freer  and  Freer,  1959;  Child, 
196 1 ;  Pitelka  and  Child,  1964),  many  investigators  believe 
that  paddle  cilia  are  genuine  organelles.  Various  functions 
have  been  proposed  for  paddle  cilia,  including  serving  as 
micro-spatulae  for  application  of  adhesive  material  or  se- 
cretions to  the  substrate  (Tamarin  el  al.,  1974;  Dilly, 
1977b),  increasing  the  efficiency  of  the  power  stroke  and 
the  effectiveness  of  water  and  feeding  currents  (Bergquist 
el  al,  1977;  Dilly,  1977a;  Arnold  and  Williams-Arnold, 
1980),  increasing  membrane  surface  area  for  trapping  food 
particles  (Dilly,  1977a),  acting  as  chemoreceptors  (Matera 
and  Davis,  1982;  Campos  and  Mann,  1988),  and  trans- 
porting unknown  materials  along  the  cilium  from  base 
to  tip  (Dilly,  1977a,  b). 

However,  the  few  studies  that  have  carefully  compared 
fresh  living  material  to  chemically  fixed  or  quick-frozen 
material  have  concluded  that  paddle  cilia  and  discocilia 
are  artifacts  caused  by  osmotic  stress,  non-physiological 
conditions,  or  fixatives  and  fixation  additives  (Ehlers  and 
Ehlers,  1978;  Bone  el  al,  1982;  Pfannenstiel,  1982;  Niel- 
sen, 1987). 

To  examine  the  status  of  paddle  cilia  anew,  we  have 
re-investigated  a  recent  SEM  report  that  paddle  cilia  are 
genuine  structures  in  veliger  larvae  of  marine  bivalves 
(Campos  and  Mann,  1988).  We  used  high-speed  video 
and  electronic  flash  DIC  microscopy  of  living  larvae  of 
Spisula  solidissima  and  Lyrodus  pedicellatus  in  normal 
seawater  and  in  hypotonic  seawater,  together  with  light 
microscopy  and  SEM  of  larvae  fixed  in  solutions  of  dif- 
ferent osmolarities  and  composition. 

We  find  that  paddle  cilia  are  indeed  artifacts,  and  pro- 
pose a  new  mechanism  to  account  for  their  formation  in 
various  animals.  A  preliminary  account  of  this  work  has 
appeared  (Short  and  Tamm,  1989). 

Materials  and  Methods 
Organisms 

Spisula  solidissima  adults  were  obtained  from  Marine 
Resources  at  the  Marine  Biological  Laboratory  (MBL) 


466 


PADDLE  CILIA  ARE  ARTIFACTS 


467 


and  maintained  in  cold  running  seawater  (15°C)  at  the 
Environmental  Studies  Laboratory  (ESL)  of  the  Woods 
Hole  Oceanographic  Institution  (WHOI);  Lyrodus  pedi- 
cellatus  larvae  were  obtained  from  adults  maintained  at 
ESL.  Spisula  adults  were  spawned  by  dissection  of  gonads 
or  by  thermal  stimulation  at  22 °C,  and  the  larvae  reared 
following  methods  of  Gallager  and  Mann  ( 1986).  Larvae 
were  fed  monocultures  of  Isochrysis  galbana  at  a  concen- 
tration of  10,000  cells  per  ml. 

Light  microscopy  and  video  recording 

Living  veliger  larvae  of  Spisula  solidissima  and  Lyrodus 
pedicellalus  were  observed  in  slide  wells  of  normal  sea- 
water  under  Zeiss  DIG  and  phase  contrast  optics  with  a 
Dage  67  video  camera  modified  for  high  field  rates  (120, 
180,  240  Hz)  and  synchronized  with  a  strobex  flash 
(Chadwick-Helmuth).  Images  were  recorded  with  a 
GYYR  model  2051  video  recorder  allowing  still-field 
playback  and  analysis.  Films  (35  mm;  Kodak  Tech  Pan 
24 1 5 )  of  larvae  were  taken  with  Zeiss  DIG  and  phase  con- 
trast optics  using  an  Olympus  OM-2N  camera  and  an 
Olympus  T-32  electronic  flash  tube  positioned  in  the  il- 
lumination path. 

Fixation  and  scanning  electron  microscopy 

Umbo  stage  larvae  of  Spisula  and  Lyrodus  were  rinsed 
in  0.45  /urn  filtered  seawater  and  fixed  in  three  ways. 
Method  1  (Campos  and  Mann,  1988):  larvae  were  si- 
phoned from  the  culture  container  and  retained  on  a  50 
Mm  nylon  mesh  screen,  then  transferred  to  filtered  sea- 
water  and  relaxed  in  8%  (w/v)  MgCl: .  Larvae  were  con- 
centrated by  centrifugation  and  fixed  in  2.5%  glutaral- 
dehyde,  0.1  A/Na  cacodylate,  pH  7.2  (total  osmolarity  of 
409  mOsmols  as  determined  by  Wescor  5100C  vapor 
pressure  osmometer)  at  4°C  for  2  h.  Larvae  were  rinsed 
3  times  in  0.1  A/  Na  cacodylate,  0.25  A/  NaCl,  pH  7.2, 
for  30  min  each,  and  post-fixed  in  1%  OsO4, 0.19  A/ NaCl, 
0. 1  A/  Na  cacodylate  for  1  h.  Larvae  were  rinsed  in  0. 1 
A/  Na  cacodylate,  0.15  M  NaCl  and  stored  overnight  at 
4°C.  Method  2:  the  same  glutaraldehyde  solution  as  above 
was  used,  but  with  0.29  A/  NaCl  added  to  make  it  isos- 
motic  with  MBL  seawater  (920  mOsmols).  Method  3: 
concentrated  larvae  were  relaxed  in  6.82%  MgCN  and 
fixed  in  unbuffered  2.5%-  glutaraldehyde,  0.13  A/  NaCl, 
50%  seawater  (isosmotic;  920  mOsmols)  at  4°C  for 
30  min. 

For  light  microscopy,  larvae  were  observed  on  slides 
after  glutaraldehyde  fixation.  For  SEM,  fixed  larvae  were 
dehydrated  through  a  graded  ethanol  series,  critical  point 
dried  (Samdri-78A),  sputtered  with  gold  palladium  (Sam- 
sputter-2a),  and  examined  with  a  JSM-840  SEM.  Pho- 
tographs were  taken  on  Polaroid  positive-negative  film. 


Results 

Light  microscopy  of  living  and  fixed  lan'ae 

The  velum  of  Spisula  solidissima  and  Lyrodus  pedi- 
cellatus  consists  of  four  ciliary  bands:  inner  and  outer 
pretrochal  bands,  an  adoral  band,  and  a  metatrochal  band. 
The  pretrochal  bands  are  responsible  for  obtaining  food 
and  for  locomotion,  and  the  adoral  and  metatrochal  bands 
convey  food  particles  to  the  mouth.  The  pretrochal  bands 
consist  of  compound  cilia;  the  adoral  and  metatrochal 
bands  contain  simple  cilia.  Figures  1A  and  IB  are  flash 
photographs  of  living  umbo  stage  larvae  of  Spisula  (two 
weeks  old)  and  Lyrodus  (two  days  old),  respectively,  in 
normal  seawater.  No  paddle  cilia  or  vesiculated  ciliary 
membranes  are  evident  in  any  of  the  ciliary  bands  of  either 
larva.  High-speed  flash-synchronized  video  microscopy 
of  swimming  Spisula  and  Lyrodus  larvae  also  failed  to 
show  modifications  of  ciliary  structure. 

The  hypotonic  glutaraldehyde  fixative  of  Campos  and 
Mann  (1988)  (Method  1),  with  or  without  OsO4  post- 
fixation,  induced  swelling  of  the  tips  of  pretrochal  cilia  of 
Spisula  larvae  (Fig.  1C)  and  vesiculation  along  pretrochal 
ciliary  membranes  of  Lyrodus  larvae  (Fig.  ID).  The  ter- 
minal swellings  of  Spisula  cilia  and  vesiculations  along 
the  shafts  of  Lyrodus  cilia  were  about  2  ^m  in  diameter. 
Because  these  modifications  of  ciliary  structure  were  ob- 
served directly  in  fixed  larvae  by  light  microscopy,  they 
are  not  induced  by  subsequent  procedures  used  for  SEM 
and  TEM. 

Addition  of  NaCl  to  make  this  fixative  isosmotic  with 
MBL  seawater  (Method  2)  resulted  in  no  paddle  cilia  in 
Spisula  larvae,  nor  vesiculation  of  ciliary  membranes  in 
Lyrodus  larvae.  Instead,  the  cilia  appeared  uniformly 
smooth  and  cylindrical.  Similarly,  an  unbuffered  isos- 
motic glutaraldehyde  fixative  containing  50%  seawater 
(Method  3)  did  not  induce  paddle  cilia  or  vesiculations 
in  either  species  (Fig.  IE,  F). 

Treatment  of  living,  2-week-old  larvae  of  Spisula  with 
45%  seawater  (420  mOsmols)  caused  swelling  of  the  distal 
tips  of  the  pretrochal  cilia  within  2  min.  These  paddle 
cilia  resembled  those  induced  by  hypotonic  fixatives  (Fig. 
1C).  Treatment  of  Lyrodus  veligers  with  45%  seawater 
resulted  in  vesiculation  of  the  membrane  along  the  entire 
shaft  of  the  pretrochal  cilia  within  10  min.  Again,  these 
vesiculated  cilia  resembled  those  induced  by  hypotonic 
fixatives  (Fig.  ID).  The  majority  of  the  modified  cilia  in 
both  species  remained  attached  to  the  velum.  However, 
treatment  with  45%'  seawater  for  longer  times  caused  de- 
tachment and  loss  of  cilia.  Upon  transfer  of  larvae  to  100% 
seawater,  many  cilia  of  both  species  regained  their  normal 
appearance  within  5-10  min,  indicating  that  tip  swelling 
or  vesiculation  is  a  reversible  osmotic  phenomenon. 

In  a  subsequent  experiment  using  2-day-old  Spisula 
veligers,  we  found  that  45%  seawater  was  ineffective  in 
producing  paddle  cilia,  but  that  15-20%  seawater  was  re- 


468 


G.  SHORT  AND  S.  L.  TAMM 


PADDLE  CILIA  ARE  ARTIFACTS 


469 


quired  to  induce  swelling  of  the  ciliary  tips  in  these  youn- 
ger larvae.  The  paddle  cilia  were  immotile  or  only  weakly 
beating  and  were  easily  detached,  resulting  in  poor  swim- 
ming ability  of  the  larvae.  Upon  transfer  to  100%  seawater. 
many  of  the  larvae  resumed  swimming.  DIC  microscopy 
of  these  larva  showed  that  some  velar  cilia  regained  a 
normal  appearance,  but  that  others  had  detached  and  were 
missing. 

Scanning  electron  microscopy  of  larvae 

Larvae  of  Spisula  and  Lyrodus  treated  with  the  isos- 
motic  fixative  containing  50%  seawater  (Method  3)  and 
processed  for  SEM,  showed  uniformly  cylindrical  velar 
cilia  without  terminal  swellings  or  vesiculations  (Figs.  2A, 
C,  3A,  C).  However,  swollen  cilia  were  present  in  both 
species  when  fixed  by  the  hypotonic  fixative  of  Campos 
and  Mann  (1988)  (Method  1)  (Figs.  2B,  D,  3B,  D).  In 
contrast  to  light  microscopic  images  of  Method  1 -fixed 
Lyrodus  larvae  (Fig.  1  D),  those  processed  for  SEM  showed 
terminal  paddles  on  pretrochal  cilia  rather  than  vesicu- 
lation  along  the  ciliary  length  (Fig.  3B,  D).  The  modified 
cilia  induced  in  Spisula  and  Lyrodus  are  similar  to  those 
observed  by  Campos  and  Mann  (1988).  The  distal  swell- 
ings measure  1-1.15  /urn  in  diameter  in  both  species,  and 
often  result  in  fraying  of  the  compound  organelles  into 
individual  cilia.  The  paddle  cilia  observed  in  Spisula  are 
not  restricted  to  the  pretrochal  ciliary  bands:  metatrochal 
cilia  also  exhibit  terminal  swellings  in  response  to  the  hy- 
potonic fixative  of  Campos  and  Mann  (1988)  (Fig.  2B). 
However,  the  metatrochal  ciliary  blebs  measure  about  1 .0 
jtm  in  diameter  and  are  located  about  1  /urn  proximal  to 
the  ciliary  tips.  The  adoral  cilia,  in  contrast,  do  not  exhibit 
dilations  at  the  tips  (Fig.  2B). 

Discussion 

We  have  reinvestigated  the  report  by  Campos  and 
Mann  (1988)  that  paddle  cilia  and  discocilia  are  genuine 
structures  in  the  velum  of  molluscan  bivalve  larvae.  Cam- 
pos and  Mann  (1988)  did  not  examine  living  larvae,  but 
used  a  hypotonic  fixative  (409  mOsmols;  Method  1)  to 
prepare  larvae  of  Spisula  solidissima  and  Mullina  lateralis 
for  SEM. 

We  imaged  beating  velar  cilia  in  larvae  of  Spisula  so- 
lidissima and  Lyrodus  pedicellatits  by  electronic  flash  and 


high-speed  video  light  microscopy.  No  paddle  cilia  or  dis- 
cocilia were  observed  in  normal  seawater.  Other  high- 
speed video  microscopic  studies  also  have  not  found 
modified  cilia  in  living  larvae  of  Spisula,  Lyrodus,  and 
Alercenaria  (Gallager,  1988;  pers.  comm.). 

We  could  reversibly  induce  swelling  of  the  ciliary 
membrane  by  treatment  of  living  larvae  with  hypotonic 
(15-45%)  seawaters.  In  addition,  paddle  cilia  were  ob- 
served using  the  hypotonic  glutaraldehyde  fixative  of 
Campos  and  Mann  (1988)  (Method  1 ),  but  not  in  fixatives 
made  isosmotic  with  seawater  (Methods  2  and  3).  We 
therefore  conclude  that  paddle  cilia  and  discocilia  in  Spis- 
ula and  Lyrodus  are  not  genuine  structures,  but  are  ar- 
tifacts. 

Of  the  numerous  reports  of  paddle  cilia  and  discocilia 
in  various  animals  (Mecklenburg  ct  al..  1974;  Tamarin 
et  al..  1974;  Oldfield,  1975;  Bergquist  et  al..  1977;  Dilly. 
1977a,  b;  Ehlers  and  Ehlers,  1978;  Heimler,  1978;  Storch 
and  Alberti,  1978;  Arnold  and  Williams-Arnold,  1980; 
Bone  et  al..  1982;  Matera  and  Davis,  1982;  Pfannenstiel, 
1982;  Nielsen,  1987;  Campos  and  Mann,  1988;  Durfot 
c/  al..  1990),  only  a  handful  of  investigators  concluded 
that  modified  cilia  are  artifacts  (Mecklenburg  et  al..  1974; 
Ehlers  and  Ehlers,  1978;  Pfannenstiel,  1982;  Bone  et  al.. 
1982;  Nielsen,  1987).  These  investigators,  in  contrast  to 
the  others,  did  not  rely  mainly  on  fixed  material,  but  used 
fresh  living  specimens  and  compared  the  effects  of  stress 
and  various  TEM  and  SEM  preparative  procedures  on 
ciliary  structure. 

For  example,  Ehlers  and  Ehlers  ( 1 978)  found  that  living, 
untreated  marine  Turbellaria  do  not  possess  paddle  cilia 
or  discocilia,  but  that  these  structures  could  be  induced 
by  the  addition  of  certain  fixative  buffers  and  chemicals 
to  the  seawater.  Osmolality  also  influenced  the  extent  of 
paddle  cilia  formation  (Ehlers  and  Ehlers,  1978). 

Similarly,  Pfannenstiel  (1982)  did  not  observe  modified 
cilia  in  living  polychaetes,  but  could  produce  paddle  cilia 
or  discocilia  by  glutaraldehyde  and  osmium  fixatives,  or 
by  MgCl:  solutions  of  different  osmolarities.  When  MgCU- 
treated  specimens  were  returned  to  seawater,  the  modified 
cilia  regained  their  normal  cylindrical  appearance,  "re- 
vealing that  they  are  transient  structures"  (Pfannenstiel, 
1982). 

In  addition,  Bone  et  al.  (1982)  found  that  the  median 
endostylar  cilia  of  dona  usually  have  straight  tips  in  fresh 


Figure  1.  DIC  flash  photographs.  A.  Living  Spisn/a  veliger  in  normal  seawater.  No  paddle  cilia  are 
present.  Scale  bar,  20  ^m.  B.  Living  Lyrodus  veliger  in  normal  seawater.  Metachronal  waves  of  pretrochal 
cilia  circle  the  velum  (es,  cilia  in  effective  stroke;  rs,  cilia  in  recovery  stroke).  No  paddle  cilia  are  evident. 
Scale  bar,  30  /jm.  C.  Spisu/a  pretrochal  cilia  fixed  in  the  hypotonic  solution  of  Campos  and  Mann  (1988) 
(our  Method  1).  Cilia  have  paddle  tips.  Scale  bar,  10  pm.  D.  LyniiJux  pretrochal  cilia  in  the  hypotonic 
fixative  of  Campos  and  Mann  (1988)  (Method  1 ).  Vesiculation  occurs  along  the  length  of  the  ciliary  mem- 
branes. Scale  bar,  20  ^m.  E.  Spisula  pretrochal  cilia  in  isosmotic  fixative  containing  50%  seawater  (Method 
3).  No  paddle  cilia  are  present.  Scale  bar,  20  Mm.  F.  Lyrothts  pretrochal  cilia  in  isosmotic  fixative  containing 
50%  seawater  (Method  3).  No  vesiculation  of  ciliary  membranes  is  evident.  Scale  bar.  20  /jm. 


470 


G.  SHORT  AND  S.  L.  TAMM 


Figure  2.  Scanning  electron  micrographs  of  velar  cilia  in  Spisiila  solidissima  larvae.  A,  C.  Isosmotic 
fixative  containing  50%  seawater  (Method  3).  No  paddle  cilia  are  present.  B,  D.  Hypotonic  fixative  of 
Campos  and  Mann  (1988)  (Method  1).  Paddle  cilia  occur  in  pretrochal  (pt)  and  metatrochal  (mt)  bands, 
but  not  in  the  adoral  cilia  (ad).  Scale  bars:  A,  10  ^m•.  B,  10  /im;  C.  1  fim;  D.  10  urn. 


living  preparations.  However,  the  addition  of  buffered  or 
unbuffered  glutaraldehyde  fixatives  induced  rapid  coiling 
of  the  ciliary  tips,  resulting  in  many  concentric  axonemal 
coils  piled  around  each  other  within  the  ciliary  membrane. 
Coiled  ciliary  tips  were  not  observed  in  SEM  material 
that  had  been  quenched  in  liquid  nitrogen  and  freeze- 
dried. 

The  few  reports  of  paddle  cilia  or  discocilia  in  living 
preparations  in  seawater  (Heimler,  1978;  Arnold  and 
Williams-Arnold,  1980;  Matera  and  Davis,  1982)  have 
been  attributed  to  osmotic  stress,  anoxia,  or  other  non- 
physiological  conditions  in  the  microscopic  slide  chambers 
used  for  observation  (Bone  et  al.  1982;  Pfannenstiel. 
1982).  In  fact,  Matera  and  Davis  (1982)  induced  reversible 
transitions  between  paddle  cilia  and  cylindrical  cilia  by 
perfusions  of  hypotonic  and  hypertonic  solutions.  In  a 
comprehensive  review  of  the  structure  of  ciliary  bands  in 


more  than  15  phyla  of  invertebrates.  Nielsen  (1987)  re- 
ported that  paddle  cilia  and  discocilia  only  occur  "in 
specimens  which  have  not  been  treated  with  sufficient 
care."  Nielsen  (1987)  concluded  that  "until  further  evi- 
dence in  favor  of  paddle  cilia  in  unstressed  animals  has 
been  presented,  I  prefer  to  regard  these  structures  as  ar- 
tifacts." 

In  this  regard,  cell  physiologists  have  long  recognized 
that  the  ciliary  membrane  is  the  weakest  part  of  the  cilium, 
and  that  osmotic  stress  or  non-physiological  conditions 
readily  cause  coiling  or  curving  of  the  axoneme  within  a 
distal  expansion  of  the  ciliary  membrane  (Hartmann, 
1953;  Lewin  and  Meinhart.  1953;  Freer  and  Freer,  1959; 
Child,  1 96  l;Pitelka  and  Child,  1964;  Mecklenburg  et  al.. 
1 974).  For  example,  Mecklenburg  et  a/.  ( 1 974)  found  that 
moderate  heat  exposure  caused  club-shaped  vesicular 
protrusions  of  the  distal  ends  of  rabbit  tracheal  cilia.  Child 


PADDLE  CILIA  ARE  ARTIFACTS 


471 


Figure  3.  Scanning  electron  micrographs  of  velar  cilia  in  Lyrodus  pedicellatus  larvae.  A,  C.  Isosmotic 
fixative  containing  50%  seawater  (Method  3).  No  paddle  cilia  are  present.  B,  D.  Hypotonic  fixative  of 
Campos  and  Mann  ( 1988)  (Method  1 ).  Paddle  cilia  are  present.  Scale  bars:  A-C,  10  ^m;  D,  1  ftm. 


reported  that  swelling  of  isolated  sucrose-treated  cilia  of 
Tetrahymena  begins  first  at  the  tip,  and  progressively 
spreads  to  the  base,  indicating  a  "proximal-distal  reduc- 
tion in  the  strength  of  connections  between  the  axoneme 
and  the  membrane"  (Pitelka  and  Child,  1964,  p.  149). 
Various  types  of  bridges  linking  the  ciliary  or  flagellar 
membrane  to  the  outer  doublet  microtubules  have  re- 
cently been  studied  biochemically  and  by  electron  mi- 
croscopy (Dentler,  1981).  A  new  mutant  of  Chlamydom- 
onas  reinhardtii  with  disc-shaped  flagellar  tips  (loop-1) 
similar  in  appearance  to  paddle  cilia  has  recently  been 
isolated,  indicating  a  possible  genetic  defect  in  the  binding 
between  the  axoneme  and  flagellar  membrane  (Nakamura 
etai,  1990). 

It  is  commonly  observed  that  paddle  cilia  and  discocilia 
are  limited  in  distribution  within  a  given  specimen;  i.e.. 
certain  ciliary  bands  or  body  regions  exhibit  modified  cilia, 
whereas  other  types  of  cilia  in  the  same  animal  appear 


normal  (Dilly,  1977a,  b;  Ehlers  and  Ehlers,  1978;  Heimler, 
1978;  Arnold  and  Williams-Arnold,  1980;  Bone  et  ai. 
1 982;  Matera  and  Davis,  1982;  Pfannenstiel,  1982;  Cam- 
pos and  Mann,  1988;  our  results).  This  restricted  distri- 
bution of  paddle  cilia  and  discocilia  has  been  used  to  argue 
against  their  artifactual  nature,  on  the  grounds  that  arti- 
factual  production  should  effect  all  cilia  uniformly 
(Bergquist  et  ai,  1977;  Matera  and  Davis,  1982;  Campos 
and  Mann,  1988).  However,  Matera  and  Davis  (1982) 
admitted  that,  "at  the  very  least,  these  findings  imply  some 
unique  properties  of  the  tips  of  paddle  cilia,  although  they 
do  not  alone  disprove  that  the  dilations  are  artifacts." 

In  fact,  workers  in  the  field  have  long  recognized  that 
"different  cilia — even  on  the  same  organism — are  not 
equally  sensitive  to  stress  and  some  cilia  are  indeed  difficult 
to  fix  in  a  normal  shape"  (Nielsen,  1987).  Our  results  on 
differences  between  ciliary  types  of  veligers  in  response  to 
hypotonic  fixatives  supports  this  finding.  It  is  well-docu- 


472 


G.  SHORT  AND  S.  L.  TAMM 


mented  that  various  types  of  locomotory  and  sensory  cilia 
differ  in  their  lipid  and  protein  composition,  as  well  as  in 
the  kinds  of  structures  linking  the  axonemal  microtubules 
to  the  membrane  (Dentler,  1981;  Bloodgood,  1990). 
Therefore,  absence  of  paddle  cilia  or  discocilia  in  certain 
types  of  cilia  or  body  regions  of  an  animal  does  not  mean 
that  modified  cilia  observed  elsewhere  on  the  organism 
are  genuine  structures. 

The  mechanism(s)  responsible  for  the  formation  of 
paddle  cilia  and  discocilia  is  not  understood.  Our  results 
on  living  and  fixed  veligers  ofSpisula  and  Lyroilm  suggest 
that  osmotic  stress,  not  the  buffers  or  fixatives  used  for 
electron  microscopy,  is  the  cause  of  modified  cilia.  How- 
ever, Ehlers  and  Ehlers  (1978)  claimed  that  certain  buffers 
and  fixation  additives  play  an  important  role  in  generating 
modified  cilia  in  marine  turbellarians.  Surprisingly,  they 
found  that  increasing  the  osmolality  of  the  fixatives  in- 
creased the  numbers  of  paddle  cilia  formed. 

Convincing  evidence  that  osmotic  changes  themselves 
are  not  required  for  formation  of  paddle  cilia  is  Pfannen- 
stiel's  (1982)  finding  that  isotonic  MgCl2  solution  induced 
paddle  cilia  in  polychaetes.  Nevertheless,  he  also  found 
that  the  number  and  time  of  appearance  of  modified  cilia 
were  inversely  related  to  the  concentration  of  MgCl: . 

Bone  el  al.  (1982)  also  discounted  osmotic  effects  as 
the  cause  of  coiling  of  ciliary  tips  in  Ciona,  because  the 
total  osmolarity  of  their  glutaraldehyde  fixatives  was 
greater  than  that  of  seawater,  and  therefore  should  have 
induced  a  transient  shrinkage  preceding  fixation. 

We  propose  a  unifying  mechanism  for  the  production 
of  paddle  cilia  that  accounts  for  many  of  these  seemingly 
contradictory  findings  (Fig.  4).  We  suggest  that  the  pri- 
mary cause  of  paddle  cilia  and  discocilia  is  a  conforma- 
tional  change  of  ciliary  doublet  microtubules  that  results 
in  the  coiling  of  the  axonemal  tip  within  the  distal  mem- 
brane. Indeed,  previous  studies  indicated  that  doublet 
microtubules  have  an  intrinsic  tendency  to  coil  when  not 
constrained  within  the  axoneme  (Summers  and  Gibbons, 
1971;  Zobel,  1973),  and  that  physiological  changes  in  Ca 
concentration  or  pH  can  induce  reversible  changes  in  the 
coiling  parameters  of  isolated  doublet  microtubules  in  so- 
lution (Miki-Noumura  and  Kamiya,  1976,  1979;Takasaki 
and  Miki-Noumura,  1982).  We  recently  showed  that  in- 
creased concentrations  of  Ca,  Ba,  or  Sr  induce  sharp  cur- 
vatures of  the  distal  end  of  axonemes  in  detergent-ex- 
tracted macrocilia  of  Beroe  (Tamm  and  Tamm,  1990). 
This  tip  curling  response  is  independent  of  ATP-powered 
microtubule  sliding,  and  is  believed  to  be  caused  by  Ca/ 
Ba/Sr-induced  helical  changes  in  doublet  microtubules, 
some  of  which  are  prevented  from  sliding  (Tamm  and 
Tamm,  1990). 

In  this  regard,  many  of  the  conditions  that  induce  pad- 
dle cilia  and  discocilia  may  initially  increase  Ca  or  proton 
flux  across  the  distal  ciliary  membrane.  For  example,  hy- 
potonic  swelling  of  the  tip  of  the  ciliary  membrane,  where 


membrane-microtubule  bridges  are  the  weakest  (Child, 
1 96 1 ;  Pitelka  and  Child.  1 964),  should  increase  membrane 
tension  and  open  stretch-activated  ion  channels,  if  present 
(Guharay  and  Sachs,  1984;  Sachs,  1988).  Because  stretch- 
activated  channels  are  cation-selective,  and  some  are  Ca- 
permeable  (Christensen,  1987;  Lansman  et  al.,  1987),  a 
resulting  influx  of  Ca  or  change  in  pH  at  the  ciliary  tip 
might  induce  a  conformational  alteration  of  doublet  mi- 
crotubules that  results  in  coiling  of  the  axonemes  (Fig. 
4A).  Secondly,  certain  fixatives  or  chemicals  may  cause 
an  initial  breakdown  or  permeabilization  of  the  ciliary 
membrane,  leading  to  similar  Ca  influx  or  pH  changes 
which  also  might  trigger  conformational  changes  of  the 
axonemal  tip  (Fig.  4B).  This  pathway,  it  should  be  noted, 
would  not  require  distal  swelling  of  the  ciliary  membrane, 
and  would  account  for  cases  of  paddle  cilia  formation 
under  isosmotic  or  hyperosmotic  conditions.  Alterna- 
tively, disruption  of  the  intact  axonemal  structure  by  pro- 
teolysis  during  fixation  or  handling  may  remove  cross- 
linking  constraints  (nexin  links,  radial  spokes)  and  allow 
spontaneous  conformational  alterations  of  the  doublet 
microtubules,  resulting  in  the  coiling  of  the  axoneme  (Zo- 
bel, 1973)  (Fig.  4C).  These  three  possible  pathways  need 
not  be  mutually  exclusive;  for  example,  destruction  of 
restraining  elements  within  the  axoneme  may  facilitate 
Ca  or  proton-induced  alterations  in  microtubule  confor- 
mation. 

Indeed,  Bone  et  al.  (1982)  found  that  Ca-blocking 
agents,  such  as  Co  and  Mn,  reversibly  uncoiled  discocilia 
in  Ciona.  These  authors  concluded  that  discocilia  are 
caused  by  coiling  of  axonemes  within  the  ciliary  mem- 
brane, but  believed  that  such  conformational  changes  were 
brought  about  primarily  by  asymmetrical  contraction  of 
the  axoneme  after  cross-linking  by  glutaraldehyde. 

Regardless  of  the  precise  pathway(s)  involved,  the  novel 
feature  of  the  proposed  mechanism  is  an  induced  or  in- 
trinsic conformational  change  of  the  doublet  microtubules 
that  leads  to  coiling  of  the  tip  of  the  axoneme  (Fig.  4). 
Dilation  or  expansion  of  the  ciliary  membrane  around 
the  looped  end  of  the  axoneme  would  then  be  merely  a 
passive  secondary  effect,  and  not  the  cause  of  coiling.  Os- 
motic swelling  of  the  ciliary  membrane  is  thus  one  method 
for  triggering  an  ion  flux  that  would  induce  a  confor- 
mational change  of  the  doublet  microtubules.  but  mem- 
brane tension  itself  would  not  be  responsible  for  the  coiling 
of  the  axoneme. 

Our  theory  for  the  production  of  paddle  cilia  is  readily 
testable.  For  example,  the  swollen  membrane  at  the  cil- 
iary tip  could  be  disrupted  or  removed.  If  the  end  of 
the  axoneme  still  remained  coiled,  then  membrane  ten- 
sion is  not  responsible  for  maintenance  of  the  paddle. 
If,  on  the  other  hand,  the  distal  end  of  the  axoneme 
uncoiled  and  straightened  upon  disruption  of  the  en- 
closing membrane,  then  membrane  tension,  not  intrin- 
sic shape  changes  of  axonemal  microtubules,  is  likely 


PADDLE  CILIA  ARE  ARTIFACTS 


473 


B 


H+- 


Co' 


Figure  4.  Proposed  mechanism  of  formation  of  paddle  cilia  and  discocilia.  Three  possible  pathways  lead 
from  normal  cylindrical  ciliary  structure  (left,  straight  black  axoneme  within  ciliary  membrane)  to  an  induced 
(A.  B)  or  spontaneous  (C)  coiling  of  the  axoneme  within  a  distal  expansion  of  the  ciliary  membrane  (right). 
A.  Osmotic  swelling  by  hypotonic  solutions  or  fixatives  stretches  the  distal  membrane  and  opens  stretch- 
activated  cation  channels;  Ca  influx  or  proton  efflux  trigger  conformational  changes  of  the  axoneme.  B. 
Fixation  or  stress  initially  weakens  the  ciliary  membrane  and  allows  Ca  influx  or  proton  efflux,  resulting  in 
coiling  of  the  axoneme  as  in  A.  No  osmotic  swelling  is  necessary.  C.  Abnormal  conditions  or  fixation  lead 
to  weakening  or  destruction  of  internal  cross-linking  restraints  (nexin  links,  radial  spokes?),  allowing  spon- 
taneous conformational  alterations  of  doublet  microtubules  and  coiling  of  the  axoneme.  These  three  pathways 
need  not  be  mutually  exclusive  (see  text). 


to  be  the  cause.  Further  experiments  along  these  lines 
are  planned. 

In  conclusion,  we  believe  that  our  work,  together  with 
previous  studies,  convincingly  shows  that  discocilia  and 
paddle  cilia  are  not  genuine  structures,  but  are  artifacts. 
The  unifying  mechanism  we  propose  to  account  for  their 
formation  suggests  that  these  modifications  may  be  useful 
for  investigating  the  structural  and  mechanical  properties 
of  axonemal  microtubules,  as  well  as  the  nature  of  mi- 
crotubule-membrane  interactions  in  cilia. 

Acknowledgments 

We  thank  Scott  Gallager.  WHOI,  for  use  of  facilities, 
technical  assistance,  and  helpful  discussion  and  advice. 


We  are  also  grateful  to  Louie  Kerr,  MBL  EM  facility,  for 
SEM  assistance,  and  to  Ms.  Dorothy  Hahn  for  patiently 
and  skillfully  processing  these  words.  This  work  was  sup- 
ported by  a  Woods  Hole  Marine  Science  Consortium  fel- 
lowship to  G.S.  and  NIH  Grant  GM27903  to  S.L.T. 

Literature  Cited 

Arnold,  J.  M.,  and  I..  D.  Williams-Arnold.  1980.  Development  of  the 
ciliature  pattern  on  the  embryo  of  the  squid  Loligo  pcak'i:  a  scanning 
electron  microscope  study.  Biot.  Bull  159:  102-1  16. 

Bergquist,  P.  R.,  C.  R.  Green,  M.  E.  Sinclair,  and  H.  S.  Roberts. 

1977.     The  morphology  of  cilia  in  sponge  larvae.  Tissue  Cell  9:  179- 
184. 

Bloodgood,  R.  A.  1990.  Ci/itiry  ami  Flagellar  Membranes.  Plenum 
Press,  New  York.  43 1  pp. 


474 


G.  SHORT  AND  S.  L.  TAMM 


Bone.  Q.,  K.  P.  Ryan,  and  A.  Pulsford.  1982.  The  nature  of  complex 
discocilia  in  the  endostyle  ofCiuna  (Tunicata:  Ascidiacea).  Mikros- 
kopu  (Wein  39:  149-153. 

Campos,  B.,  and  R.  Mann.  1988.     Discocilia  and  paddle  cilia  in  the 
larvae  of  Mulinia  latertilisand  Spiuila  \olitlissima  (Mollusca:  Bival- 
via).  Biol.  Bull.  175:  343-348. 
Child,  F.  M.  1961.     Some  aspects  of  the  chemistr>'  of  cilia  and  flagella. 

i:\l>  Cell  Res.  Suppl.  8:  47-53. 
Christensen,  O.  1987.     Mediation  of  cell  volume  regulation  by  Ca2+ 

influx  through  stretch-activated  channels.  Nature  330:  66-68. 
Dentler,  \V.  L.  1981.     Microtuhule-memhrane  interactions  in  cilia  and 

flagella.  Inl.  Rev.  Cylol  72:  1-47. 
Dilly,  P.  N.  1977a.     Material  transport  within  specialized  ciliary  shafts 

on  Rhabdoplt'lira  zooids.  Cell  Tissue  Rex.  180:  367-381. 
Dilly,  P.  N.  1977b.     Further  observations  of  transport  within  paddle 

cilia.  Cell  Tissue  Res.  185:  105-1 13. 

Durfot,  M.,  E.  Sagrista,  M.  G.  Bozzo,  M.  Poquet,  J.  Garcia  Valero, 
M.  J.  Amor,  J.  Ferrer,  and  E.  Ribes.  1990.  Modified  cilia  in  vibratil 
epithelia  of  mussels  infected  by  Mytilieola  mtestinalis  and  Modiolicola 
gracilis  (Crustacea,  Copepoda).  4th  Internal.  Colloq.  Palhol.  Marine 
Aquacul..  Vigo.  Spain.  Pp.  108-109. 
Ehlers,  V..  and  B.  Ehlers.  1978.  Paddle  cilia  and  discociha— genuine 

structures?  Cell  Tissue  Res.  192:  489-501. 

Gallager,  S.  M.  1988.  Visual  observations  of  particle  manipulation 
during  feeding  in  larvae  of  a  bivalve  mollusc.  Bull.  Mar.  Sci.  43:  344- 
365. 

Gallager,  S.  M.,  and  R.  Mann.  1986.  Growth  and  survival  of  Mercc- 
naria  mercenaria  (L.)  and  Crassoslrea  Virginia  (Gmelin)  relative  to 
broodstock  conditioning  and  lipid  content  of  eggs.  Aqiiacii/lure  56: 
105-121. 

Guharay,  F.,  and  F.  Sachs.  1984.     Stretch-activated  single  ion  channel 
currents  in  tissue-cultured  embryonic  chick  skeletal  muscle.  J.  Phys- 
iol..  Land.  352:685-701. 
Hartmann,  M.  1953.     Allgememe  Biologic.  Gustav  Fischer  Verlag, 

Stuttgart.  P.  143. 

Heimler,  \V.  1978.     Discocilia— a  new  type  of  kinocilia  in  the  larvae 
of  Lamce  conchilega  (Polychaeta.  Terebellomorpha).  Cell  Tissue  Res. 
187:271-280. 
Koltzoff,  N.  1953.     Abschnitt.  Dynamik.  Pp.  140-143  in  Allgemeinc 

Biologie.  M.  Hartmann.  ed.  Gustav  Fischer  Verlag.  Stuttgart. 
Lansman,  J.  B.,  Hallam,  T.  J.,  and  I.  J.  Rink.  1987.     Single  stretch- 
activated  ion  channels  in  vascular  endothelial  cells  as  mechanotrans- 
ducers?  Nature  325:  811-813. 

Lewin,  R.  A.,  and  J.  O.  Meinhart.  1953.  Studies  on  the  flagella  of 
algae.  III.  Electron  micrographs  of  Chlamydomonas  moewusii.  Can 
J  Bot.  31:  711-717. 

Matera,  E.  M.,  and  W.  J.  Davis.  1982.  Paddle  cilia  (discocilia)  in  che- 
mosensitive  structures  of  the  gastropod  mollusk  Pleurobranchaea 
califomica.  Cell  Tissue  Res  222:  25-40. 


Mecklenburg,  C.  V.,  V.  Mercke,  C.  H.  Hakansson,  and  N.  G.  Toremalm. 
1974.     Morphological  changes  in  ciliary  cells  due  to  heat  exposure. 
((•//  TUMIC  Res   148:  45-56. 
Miki-lSoumura,  I.,  and  R.  Kamiya.  1976.     Shape  of  microtubules  in 

solutions.  E.\T-  Cell  Res  97:  451-453. 

Miki-Noumura,  T.,  and  R.  Kamiya.  1979.  Conformational  change  in 
the  outer  doublet  microtubules  from  sea  urchin  sperm  flagella.  ./ 
Cell  Biol.  81:  355-360. 

Nakaimira.  S.,  M.  \\atanabe,  K.  Hatase,  and  M.  K.  Kojima.  1990.  Light 
inhibits  flagellation  in  a  Chlamydomonas  mutant.  Plant  Cell  Physiol. 
31:  349-401. 

Nielsen,  C.  1987.  Structure  and  function  of  metazoan  ciliary  bands 
and  their  phylogenetic  significance.  Ada  Zoo/.  (Stockh.)  68:  205- 
262. 

Oldfield,  S.  C.  1975.     Surface  fine  structure  of  the  globiferous  pedicel- 
lariae  of  the  regular  echinoid,  Psammechinus  nuliaris  (Gmelin).  Cell 
n^uc  Res.  162:  377-385. 
Pfannenstiel,  H.  D.  1982.     Modified  axonemes  and  ciliary  membranes 

in  three  polychaete  species.  Cell  Tissue  Res.  224:  181-188. 
Pitelka,  D.  R.,  and  F.  M.  Child.  1964.     The  locomotor  apparatus  of 
ciliatesand  flagellates:  relations  between  structure  and  function.  Pp. 
131-198  in  Biochemistry  and  Physiology  of  Protozoa,  vol.  III.  S.  H. 
Hutner,  ed.  Academic  Press.  New  York. 

Freer,  J.  R.,  and  I..  B.  Preer.  1959.     Gel  diffusion  studies  on  the  antigens 
of  isolated  cellular  components  of  Paramecium.  J  Protozool.  6:  88- 
100. 
Sachs,  F.  1988.     Mechanical  transduction  in  biological  systems.  CRC 

Crn  Re\:  B  Joined.  Eng.  16:  141-169. 

Short,  G.,  and  S.  L.  Tamm.  1989.     Paddle  cilia  occur  as  artifacts  in 
veliger  larvae  of  Spisula  solidissima  and  Lyrodes  pedicellatus.  Biol. 
Bull.  177:  314. 
Storch,  V.,  and  G.  Albert!.  1978.     Ultrastructural  observations  on  the 

gills  of  polychaetes.  Helgol.  Wiss.  Meeresunters.  31:  169-179. 
Summers,  K.  E.,  and  I.  R.  Gibbons.  1971.     Adenosine  triphosphate- 
induced  sliding  of  tubules  in  trypsin-treated  flagella  of  sea-urchin 
sperm.  Proc.  Nail.  Acad.  Sci.  USA  68:  3092-3096. 
Takasaki,  V.,  and  T.  Miki-Noumura.  1982.     Shape  of  the  ciliary  doublet 

microtubule  in  solution.  /  Mol.  Biol.  158:  317-324. 
Tamarin,  A.,  P.  Lewis,  and  J.  Askey.  1974.     Specialized  cilia  of  the 
byssus  attachment  plaque  forming  region  in  Mytilus  calilornianus 
.1  Morphol.  142:321-327. 
Lamm,  S.  I..,  and  S.  Tamm.  1990.     Ca/Ba/Sr-induced  conformational 

changes  of  ciliary  axonemes.  Cell  Motil.  Cytoskel.  17:  187-196. 
Zobel,  C.  R.  1973.     Eftect  of  solution  composition  and  proteolysis  on 
the  conformation  of  axonemal  components.  /  Cell  Biol.  59:  573- 
594. 


Reference:  Biol.  Bull  180:  475-484.  (June,  1WI ) 


Metabolism  and  Excretion  of  Injected  [3H]-Ecdysone 
by  Female  Lobsters,  Homarus  americanus 

MARK  J.  SNYDER1   AND  ERNEST  S.  CHANG: 

Bodega  Marine  Laboratory,  University  of  California,  P.O.  Box  247,  Bodega  Bay,  California  94923 


Abstract.  The  dynamics  of  ecdysteroid  metabolism  and 
excretion  were  followed  in  adult  lobsters.  Homarus  amer- 
icanus. Females  at  five  different  molt  stages  were  injected 
with  [3H]-ecdysone.  Levels  of  [3H]-20-hydroxyecdysone 
(20E),  converted  from  [3H]-ecdysone,  rose  rapidly  and 
remained  significantly  higher  in  premolt  stages  D0  and 
D, .  In  contrast,  significant  increases  in  the  levels  of  highly 
polar  ecdysteroid  metabolites  (HP)  occurred  primarily  in 
stages  A  and  C.  Changes  in  the  hemolymph  levels  of  20E 
and  HP  in  hemolymph  over  the  molt  cycle  suggest  ad- 
ditional metabolic  mechanisms  by  which  the  liters  of  ac- 
tive molting  hormones  can  be  regulated. 

Excretion  of  [3H]-ecdysleroids  was  slower  during  early 
premolt  stages  D0  and  D, .  suggesting  that  this  reduced 
rate  may  be  an  additional  mechanism  for  regulating  ec- 
dysteroid titers.  Study  of  [3H]-ecdysteroids  indicated  that 
metabolism  proceeds  primarily  to  HP  that  are  excreted 
in  the  urine  with  unaltered  ecdysteroids.  An  additional 
ecdysteroid  metabolic  route  was  found  in  the  midgut 
gland;  this  route  removes  ecdysteroids  from  the  hemo- 
lymph and  transforms  them  into  apolar  metabolites  prior 
to  their  excretion  in  the  feces.  This  route  is  similar  to  that 
previously  found  for  ingested  [3H]-ecdysone,  which  was 
converted  to  apolar  conjugates  without  further  absorption. 

Introduction 

The  first  ecdysteroid  isolated  from  a  decapod  crustacean 
was  20-hydroxyecdysone  (20E)  by  Hampshire  and  Horn 


Received  2  October  1990;  accepted  5  February  1991. 

1  Current  address:  Department  of  Entomology,  University  of  Arizona, 
Tucson,  AZ  85721. 

2  To  whom  all  correspondence  should  be  addressed. 

Abbreviations:  20.26E,  20.26-dihydroxyecdysone;  20E,  20-hydroxy- 
ecdysone; 20EA,  20-hydroxyecdysonoic  acid;  HP,  highly  polar  ecdysteroid 
metabolites;  HPLC,  high-performance  liquid  chromatography;  P.  pona- 
sterone  A:  RP.  reverse  phase:  T,  triol  (22,25-dideoxyecdysone). 


( 1966).  Since  that  report,  nearly  20  different  ecdysteroids 
have  been  identified  from  over  25  crustacean  species  (see 
Chang.  1989,  for  review).  20E  has  been  reported  to  be  the 
putative  active  molting  hormone,  because  it  specifically 
alters  premolt  changes  in  kinase  activities  and  protein 
synthesis  in  epidermal  tissues  (Christ  and  Sedlmeier,  1987; 
Traubel  al..  1987). 

The  primary  ecdysteroid  product  of  the  molting  gland, 
or  Y-organ.  is  thought  to  be  ecdysone  (Chang  and  O'Con- 
nor, 1977).  Additional  evidence  suggests  that  the  Y-organ 
secretes  other  ecdysteroids,  namely  25-deoxyecdysone 
(Lachaise  et  ai,  1989)  and  3-dehydroecdysone  (Spaziani 
et  al..  1989).  A  single  hydroxylation  step  converts  ecdy- 
sone and  25-deoxyecdysone  to  the  more  active  products 
20E  and  ponasterone  A  (P),  respectively.  Further  metab- 
olism proceeds  by  additional  hydroxylation  steps,  for- 
mation of  acids,  and  conjugation  to  form  polar  and  apolar 
products  (McCarthy,  1980,  1982;  Lachaise  and  Lafont, 
1984;  Connat  and  Diehl,  1986;  Snyder  and  Chang. 
1991a.  b). 

Many  decapod  tissues  absorb  [3H]-ecdysone  or  [3H]- 
20E  from  the  hemolymph  (Kuppert  et  al.,  1978;  Mc- 
Carthy, 1980,  1982)  and  metabolize  these  injected  ecdy- 
steroids in  vitro  ( Lachaise  and  Lafont,  1984).  The  metab- 
olism of  ecdysteroids  has  been  studied  in  greater  detail  in 
insects,  and  the  structural  identities  of  many  metabolites 
have  been  confirmed  by  mass  spectrometry,  nuclear  mag- 
netic resonance,  and  other  chemical  techniques  (reviewed 
by  Koolman  and  Karlson,  1985). 

A  characteristic  pattern  of  hemolymph  ecdysteroid  ti- 
ters defines  the  crustacean  molt  cycle;  i.e.,  titers  are  low 
until  the  final  large  premolt  peak,  and  this  peak  is  followed 
by  a  rapid  decline  just  prior  to  ecdysis  (Chang,  1989). 
Recently,  the  decapods  Uca  pitgilator  and  Homarus 
americanus  were  reported  as  having  other  significant  liter 
variations  during  their  moll  cycles  (Hopkins,  1986;  Snyder 
and  Chang,  199  la).  Hemolymph  20E  levels  in  Ihe  lobster 


475 


476 


M.  J.  SNYDER  AND  E.  S.  CHANG 


//.  iiincricanus  drop  precipitously  in  late  premolt,  and  the 
drop  is  associated  with  an  increase  in  the  liter  of  highly 
polar  metabolites  (Snyder  and  Chang,  199 la).  The  liters 
of  hemolymph  ecdysteroids  decrease  bolh  in  lale  premoll 
and  when  regeneraling  limb  buds  aulolomize.  These 
changes  are  explicable  by  increases  in  bolh  the  metabolism 
of  ecdysteroids  lo  polar  conjugales  and  Ihe  excrelion  of 
ecdysteroids  ( McCarthy,  1980,  1982).  Other  than  control 
of  the  Y-organ  by  molt-inhibiting  hormone,  addilional 
conlrolling  mechanisms  for  Ihe  regulalion  of  ecdysleroid 
lilers  are  lillle  known  in  cruslaceans  (Chang,  1989). 

Excretion  palhways  for  ecdysleroids  in  cruslaceans  have 
received  lillle  altenlion.  When  decapods  were  injecled 
wilh  [3H]-ecdysone,  [3H]-20E,  or  [3H]-P,  much  of  Ihe  ra- 
diolabel  appeared  in  Ihe  surrounding  waler  wilhin  1  to 
48h(Lachaisef/rt/..  1976;  Kuppert  et  al.,  1978;  Buchholz. 
1982;  Lachaise  and  Lafonl,  1984).  We  recenlly  cannulated 
both  the  antennal  gland  and  anus  and  found  lhal  ecdy- 
sleroids are  excreled  bolh  in  urine  and  feces,  allhough 
urine  is  Ihe  major  roule  (Snyder  and  Chang,  1991b).  The 
gul  also  excretes  ecdysleroids  from  Ihe  hemolymph,  in 
addition  to  playing  an  important  role  in  detoxifying  (by 
apolar  conjugalion)  and  excreling  ingesled  ecdysteroids 
(Snyder  and  Chang,  1991b).  That  Ihe  gul  melabolizes  ec- 
dysleroids, whelher  ingesled,  endogenous,  or  injecled,  has 
been  found  in  several  arthropods  (Isaac  and  Slinger,  1989). 

We  have  injecled  ecdysleroids  into  lobslers  al  five  slages 
in  Ihe  moll  cycle,  and  have  delermined  Ihe  levels  of  me- 
laboliles  produced.  These  sludies  have  revealed  the 
changes  in  ecdysteroid  metabolism  lhal  occur  during  Ihe 
moll  cycle.  In  addilion,  we  have  cannulaled  the  anus  and 
urinary  pores,  collecled  Ihe  radiolabeled  metabolites,  and 
thus  elucidated  the  excretory  roules  for  injecled  ecdyste- 
roids. 


Materials  and  Methods 


Animals 


Adult  female  Homanis  americamts  (420-570  g  wet  wt.) 
were  eilher  oblained  from  a  seafood  supplier  (Nel  Result 
Martha's  Vineyard,  Massachuselts)  or  were  reared  al  Ihe 
Bodega  Marine  Laboratory  (Chang  and  Conklin,  1983; 
Conklin  and  Chang,  1983).  No  differences  were  observed 
belween  the  lobslers  oblained  from  Ihese  Iwo  sources. 
Only  non-reproduclive  lobslers  were  used  in  Ihis  sludy  to 
avoid  ovarian  influences  on  ecdysleroid  dynamics  (La- 
chaise  et  al.,  1981).  They  were  mainlained  in  a  flow- 
through  system  al  12  ±  3.5°C  on  a  16L:8D  pholoperiod 
and  fed  a  mixed  diel  of  frozen  fish,  shrimp,  and  live  mus- 
sels Ihrice  weekly.  The  lobsler  premoll  slages  D0' °,  D',, 
D",  D'i",  D':,  and  D,  of  Aiken  (1973)  are  reported  here  as 
stages  Do1,  D,1,  D,2,  D,\  D:',  and  D2\  respectively.  Slag- 
ing  of  postmoll  and  early  inlermoll  was  made  according 


lo  Ihe  degree  of  softness  of  the  carapace  and  chelae  as 
reported  by  Slevenson  (1968). 

Cannulalion  was  accomplished  as  follows.  A  lobsler 
was  reslrained  on  ils  dorsum  on  a  bed  of  ice.  Bolh  antennal 
gland  pores  were  then  exlernally  cannulaled  by  the  meth- 
ods of  Holliday  ( 1977).  In  addition,  a  cannula  was  inserted 
into  Ihe  anus  and  held  in  place  wilh  cyanomethacrylate 
glue.  The  cannula  (3. 1 8  X  4.76  X  80  mm,  i.d.  X  o.d.  X  1 ) 
was  open-ended  and  initially  filled  with  air.  When  properly 
allached,  water  did  nol  enler  Ihe  cannula.  When  Ihe  lob- 
sler defecaled,  Ihe  feces  expelled  air  dislally  from  the  can- 
nula. The  remaining  air  in  Ihe  cannula  prolecled  the  feces 
from  being  conlaminaled  by  seawaler.  Feces  were  col- 
lecled from  Ihe  cannula  after  il  was  removed  from  Ihe 
animal. 

Injections 

[23,24-'H]-ecdysone  (89  Ci/mmol,  New  England  Nu- 
clear) was  purified  by  high-performance  liquid  chroma- 
tography  (HPLC),  dissolved  in  lobster  saline  (Mykles. 
1980),  and  3-4  ^Ci  injected  into  the  hemocoel  al  Ihe  base 
of  Ihe  fourth  pereiopod.  The  injecled  ecdysone  did  nol 
raise  Ihe  levels  of  circulating  ecdysleroids  above  Ihose 
previously  observed  (Snyder  and  Chang,  199 la).  Injecled 
animals  were  in  slages  A-B,  C4,  D0',  D,1,  and  D22-D3'. 
The  hemolymph  was  sampled  al  1,4,  12,  24,  48,  72,  and 
96  h  after  injeclion,  and  all  excrela  were  collecled  daily. 
Each  of  Ihe  collected  samples  was  extracled  in  melhanol 
and  prepared  for  HPLC  and  liquid  scinlillalion  spec- 
Iromelry,  as  described  previously  (Snyder  and  Chang, 
1991a,b). 

In  one  experiment,  juvenile  female  lobsters  (stage  C4, 
29_46  gwelwt.)  were  injecled  wilh  1  /jCiof[3H]-ecdysone. 
Al  1  h  and  10  days,  four  lobslers  were  sacrificed,  and  Ihe 
midgul  glands,  ovaries,  abdominal  muscles,  hindguls,  an- 
lennal  glands,  epidermal  tissues  of  the  cephalolhorax,  and 
Ihe  remaining  carcasses  were  extracted  in  100%  methanol. 
Following  two  re-extraclions  and  cenlrifugalions  (10  min, 
4 1 00  X  g),  portions  of  Ihe  resullanl  supernalanls  were 
subjecled  lo  liquid  scinlillalion  speclromelry  for  the  de- 
termination of  lolal  radioaclivity  per  lissue.  Because  Ihe 
sample  exlracls  were  highly  diluled,  no  variations  in 
counting  efficiency  were  observed.  As  a  positive  control, 
we  added  [3H]-ecdysone  to  non-radiolabeled  tissues  prior 
to  Ihe  exlraclion  sleps  and  Ihus  delermined  lhal  our  tissue 
exlraclion  efficiencies  were  80-90%. 

HPLC 

Samples  of  individual  hemolymph,  urine,  and  fecal  ex- 
tracts  were  dissolved  in  the  appropriate  solvent,  centri- 
fuged,  and  Ihe  supernalanl  injecled  direclly  onto  a  Walers 
C|8  juBondapak  column  (3.9  mm  I.D.  X  30  cm).  One  of 
Ihe  following  reverse  phase  elulion  condilions  was  used: 


LOBSTER  ECDYSTEROID  METABOLISM 


477 


100 


>s 

•o 
o 

LjJ 

-C 

a 

^ 
o 

E 


0) 
0- 


12      24      36      48      60      72      84      96 
Time  After  Injection  (h) 


Figure  1.  Changes  in  the  hemolymph  level  of  injected  ['H]-ecdysone 
(E)  as  a  function  of  time  and  molt  stage.  [3H]-ecdysone  was  injected  at 
time  zero.  Concentration  of  labeled  ecdysone  in  the  hemolymph  ot  lob- 
sters is  expressed  as  a  percentage  of  the  total  [3H]-ecdysteroids,  and  de- 
termined by  methanolic  extraction  of  hemolymph  samples  followed  by 
scintillation  counting  of  reverse  phase-HPLC  fractions.  Samples  were 
separated  with  gradient  systems  #1  or  #2  (see  text)  with  either  (or  both, 
for  some  samples)  methanol  or  acetonitrile  as  the  solvent.  Molt  stages 
A.  C,  D0,  D,,  and  D2  refer  to  the  morphological  designations  of  Aiken 
(1973).  Sample  sizes  were  as  given  for  Table  I  with  the  addition  of  n  =  3 
for  stage  A.  Standard  deviation  bars  are  omitted  for  clarity. 


( 1 )  a  35  min  linear  gradient  of  20-100%  methanol  in  water 
at  1.0  ml/min  (1.0  min  fractions);  (2)  a  linear  gradient  of 
20-100%  acetonitrile  in  20  mM  Tris,  pH  7.5,  at  1.0  ml/ 
min  (1.0  min  fractions  collected);  or  (3)  a  linear  gradient 
of  20-100%  methanol  in  20  m.l/Tris,  pH  7.5,  at  1.0  ml/ 
min  ( 1 .0  min  fractions  collected).  In  all  cases,  we  employed 
a  Waters  HPLC  system.  Duplicate  samples  from  each 
fraction  were  analyzed  by  scintillation  spectrometry.  The 
sum  of  the  radioactive  ecdysteroids  recovered  in  the 
HPLC-fractions  was  equal  to  70-85%  of  the  total,  un- 
fractionated  radioactivity.  The  amount  of  each  [3H]-ec- 
dysteroid  metabolite  was  expressed  as  the  percentage  of 
the  total  radioactivity,  and  the  values  at  each  time  point 
were  compared  statistically  by  ANOVA  and  Scheffe  tests 
of  arcsine  transformed  values  (Sokal  and  Rohlf,  1969). 

Enzymatic  hydrolysis 

The  fractions  resulting  from  HPLC  that  contained  ec- 
dysteroids of  greater  polarity  than  20E  are  designated 
"polar  fractions."  The  polar  fractions  from  individual 
samples  of  urine  and  feces  were  pooled  and  then  incu- 
bated, at  37°C  for  24  h.  in  1.0  ml  sodium  acetate  buffer 
(50  mM,  pH  5.5)  containing  3.0  mg/ml  type  H-2  Helix 
pomatia  sulfatase  (Sigma).  Apolar  fractions  from  fecal 
samples  were  dissolved  in  ethanol  (5%  v/v  in  final  hydro- 
lysis mixture)  with  or  without  addition  of  enzymes,  and 
incubated  for  72  h  (Whiting  and  Dinan,  1988).  These 


modifications  increased  the  hydrolysis  of  apolar  material 
(Whiting  and  Dinan,  1988;  Snyder  and  Chang,  1991b). 
After  the  addition  of  three  volumes  of  methanol  to  ter- 
minate the  reactions,  the  samples  were  centrifuged  at  4 1 00 
X  g.  re-extracted  twice,  and  the  pooled  supernatants 
evaporated  under  reduced  pressure  and  analyzed  by 
HPLC-scintillation  spectrometry. 

Results 

Hemolymph  ecdysteroids 

Changes  in  hemolymph  ecdysteroid  metabolites  were 
followed  for  96  h  after  the  injection  of  [3H]-ecdysone. 
Figure  1  shows  the  rate  of  disappearance  of  ecdysone  from 
the  hemolymph.  The  loss  of  ecdysone,  as  a  percentage  of 
the  total  hemolymph  [3H]-ecdysteroids,  was  not  signifi- 
cantly different  from  one  molt  stage  to  another.  Within 
1  h,  ecdysone  levels  had  fallen  to  about  70%  of  the  total. 
Levels  dropped  dramatically  to  6.5- 1 2.2%  by  24  h.  Levels 
of  [3H]-ecdysone  did  not  fall  to  zero  and  were  still  2.5- 
5.5%  of  the  total  at  96  h. 

Hemolymph  [3H]-20E  levels  were  also  followed  after 
the  injection  of  [3H]-ecdysone  (Fig.  2).  By  1  h,  [3H]-20E 
percentages  were  17-27%.  of  the  total  [3H]-ecdysteroids 
and  not  significantly  different  among  the  different  molt 
stages.  At  4  and  12  h,  lobsters  in  stages  D0  and  D,  had 
higher  percentages  of  labeled  20E  (relative  to  other  ec- 
dysteroids) than  at  other  molt  stages.  Levels  of  20E  for 
both  stages  (>75%  of  the  total)  were  consistently  higher 


20E 


100n 


Q. 

E 

_>N 

o 

V 

x 


tf  0        12       24      36       48       60       72      84      96 

u 

D. 

Time  After  Injection  (h) 

Figure  2.  Change  in  the  hemolymph  level  of  radiolabeled  20-hy- 
droxyecdysone  (20E)  as  a  function  of  time  and  molt  stage.  ['H]-ecdysone 
was  injected  at  time  zero.  Concentration  of  labeled  20E  in  the  hemolymph 
of  lobsters  is  expressed  as  a  percentage  of  the  total  [3H]-ecdysteroids. 
Separation  and  quantification  conditions  are  as  listed  in  Figure  1  and 
Materials  and  Methods.  Sample  sizes  are  as  given  for  Table  I  with  the 
addition  of  n  =  3  for  stage  A.  Bars  indicate  one  standard  deviation  from 
the  mean. 


478 


M.  J.  SNYDER  AND  E.  S.  CHANG 


T3 
O 
LJ 

-C 

a 

^v 

o 
E 

0) 


o 

•*-* 

c 


tu 
Q. 


100n 


75- 


0        12      24      36      48      60      72      84      96 
Time  After  Injection  (h) 


Figure  3.  Change  in  the  hemolymph  level  of  radiolabeled  highly 
polar  ecdysteroid  metabolites  (HP)  as  a  function  of  time  and  molt  stage. 
['H]-ecdysone  was  injected  into  lobsters  at  time  zero.  Concentration  of 
labeled  HP  in  the  hemolymph  is  expressed  as  a  percentage  of  the  total 
['H]-ecdysteroids.  Separation  and  quantification  conditions  are  as  listed 
in  Figure  I  and  Materials  and  Methods.  Sample  sizes  are  as  given  for 
Table  I  with  the  addition  of  n  =  3  for  stage  A.  Bars  indicate  one  standard 
deviation  from  the  mean. 


than  those  of  other  stages  through  96  h.  Premolt  lobsters 
in  stages  D,  and  D2  had  similar  20E  percentages  at  least 
until  12  h  post-injection  of  [3H]-ecdysone.  By  24  h,  the 
[3H]-20E  in  the  D2  lobsters  started  to  drop  dramatically, 
reaching,  by  72-96  h,  levels  equivalent  to  those  in  stages 
A  and  C.  Levels  of  [3H]-20E  were  significantly  lower  in 
D:  than  either  D0  or  D,  lobsters  by  24  h.  The  concentra- 
tions of  [3H]-20E  in  stages  A  and  C  changed  together 
throughout  the  96  h  experiment.  Levels  for  those  two 
stages  peaked  (41-47%)  at  4  h,  and  then  dropped  to  7.9- 
11.1%  of  the  total  [3H]-ecdysteroids  by  72  h.  Stage  A  and 
C  [3H]-20E  levels  were  significantly  lower  than  those  of 
all  other  stages  until  96  h  post-injection,  when  stage  D: 
lobsters  had  similar  values.  These  data  indicate  that  the 
rate  of  20E  loss  becomes  significantly  faster  as  lobsters 
approach  stage  D, . 

Changes  with  time  in  the  percentages  of  highly  polar 
ecdysteroid  metabolites  (HP)  were  the  converse  of  those 
in  20E  (Fig.  3).  All  molt  stages  were  similar  from  1-4  h 
post-injection  of  [3H]-ecdysone.  The  hemolymph  per- 
centages of  [3H]-HP  in  stages  D0  and  D,  lobsters  barely 
increased.  This  is  because  [3H]-20E  percentages  remain 
high  in  these  two  stages  through  96  h  (Fig.  2).  The  per- 
centage of  [3H]-HP  in  late  premolt  stage  D:  lobsters  was 
equivalent  to  those  in  D0  and  D,  animals  through  12  h 
(8-17%  of  the  total),  then  rose  to  a  significantly  higher 
level  by  48  h.  At  72  h,  stages  D:,  A,  and  C  had  similar 
[3H]-HP  percentages.  Stage  A  and  C  lobsters  both  showed 
rapid  increases  in  [3H]-HP  percentages  that  were  signifi- 
cantly higher  (increasing  to  >75%  of  the  total)  than  those 
in  other  molt  stages. 


Ecd\  -steroid  e.\cn't  ion 

Table  I  gives  the  data  for  total  excretion  of  [3H]-ecdy- 
steroids,  as  a  percentage  of  the  injected  dose,  during  the 
first  6  or  the  first  1 5  days  after  injection  of  [3H]-ecdysone. 
Lobsters  in  stages  D,,  and  D,  excreted  significantly  less 
ecdysteroids  (12%.)  in  the  first  6  days  after  injection  than 
those  in  stages  C  or  D2  (30%).  By  1 5  days,  D0  lobsters  had 
excreted  significantly  less  [3H]-ecdysteroids  (28%)  than 
those  in  stage  D,  (39%)  and  C  (45%).  Urine  was  always 
the  major  route  for  ecdysteroid  excretion  (Table  I).  In 
stages  C  and  D: ,  90-96%  of  the  radiolabel  was  excreted 
in  the  urine.  Significantly  more  [3H]-ecdysteroids  were 
excreted  in  the  feces  of  stage  D0  and  D,  lobsters  than  at 
stages  C  or  D: . 

Ecdysteroid  metabolites 

Lobsters  in  stages  C  through  D,  were  injected  with  [3H]- 
ecdysone.  Ninety-six  hours  later,  the  profiles  of  ecdysteroid 
metabolites  in  hemolymph,  urine,  and  feces  were  exam- 
ined. The  data  for  injections  into  stages  C  (Fig.  4)  and  D, 
(Fig.  5)  were  similar  to  those  for  injections  into  stages  D: 
and  D(l,  respectively.  Therefore,  profiles  for  the  latter  in- 
jections are  not  shown.  At  least  four  HP  were  resolved  by 
RP-HPLC.  Besides  20E,  ecdysone,  and  P,  two  of  these 
HP  were  tentatively  identified  as  epimers  of  20-hydroxy- 
ecdysonoic  acid  (20EA)  and  20,26-dihydroxyecdysone 
(20.26E).  The  characterizations  were  based  on  co-elution 
with  authentic  standards  in  at  least  two  different  solvent 
systems;  normal  phase  separations  were  also  performed 
on  some  samples  (data  not  shown).  In  addition,  two  other 
ecdysteroid  metabolites  were  designated  as  "highly  polar 


Table  I 

Percentage  of  radioactivity  recovered  from  excreta  of  adult  lobsters 
following  injection  of[''H]-ecdysone 


Urine  plus  feces2 


Days  1-153 


Molt  stage1     n       Days  1-6       Days  1-15          Urine 


Feces 


C 

5 

30.0  ±  4.  1  J 

45.2  ±  3.7a 

90.2  ±  2.9a 

9.8  ±  2.9" 

D0 

3 

11.5  ±  2.8" 

28.2  ±  4.7" 

80.6  ±  0.8" 

19.4  ±0.8" 

D, 

4 

12.0±  2.1h 

38.7  ±  5.0a 

73.3  ±  3.5C 

26.7  ±  3.5" 

D: 

4 

30.6  ±  3.9a 

— 

95.5  ±  2.1d 

4.5  ±2.1" 

1  Molt  stage  designations  are  those  of  Aiken  (1973).  Postmolt  stages 
A  and  B  were  not  monitored  for  [3H]  excretion.  Values  with  different 
superscript  letters  (within  a  column)  indicate  significant  differences  (P 
<  0.05). 

2  Sum  of  the  radioactivity  excreted  in  both  the  urine  and  feces  in 
either  the  first  6  or  the  first  1 5  days  following  ['H]-ecdysone  injection. 
Late  premolt  stage  D:  lobsters  were  followed  only  for  the  first  6  days 
(after  which  they  molted). 

3  Percentages  of  total  radioactivity  recovered  in  either  the  urine  or 
feces  over  the  entire  experiment  (days  1-15). 


LOBSTER  ECDYSTEROID  METABOLISM 


479 


0. 

E 

o 
1/1 


V 

vc. 


t> 

Q- 


20  n 


10 


Hemolymph 


Urine 


40 


50  60 


Retention  Time  (min) 


Figure  4.  Reverse  phase-HPLC-scintillation  spectromelric  analyses 
ofhemolymph,  urine,  and  fecal  [3H]-ecdysteroids.  Samples  were  obtained 
from  a  stage  C4  lobster  96  h  after  injection  of  pH]-ecdysone.  Separation 
conditions  are  described  in  Figure  1 .  The  retention  times  of  authentic 
20-hydroxyecdysonoic  acid  epimers  (20EA),  20,26-dihydroxyecdysone 
(20.26E),  20-hydroxyecdysone  (20E),  ecdysone  (E),  ponasterone  A  (P), 
and  22,25-dideoxyecdysone  (triol.  T)  are  shown.  Two  highly  polar  ec- 
dysteroid  products,  which  include  conjugates  and  non-enzyme-hydro- 
lyzable  metabolites,  are  labeled  as  HP1  and  HP2. 


with  a  retention  time  similar  to  that  of  22,25-dideoxyec- 
dysone (triol,  T).  Only  small  percentages  of  [3H]-20E,  ec- 
dysone, and  P  were  ever  found  in  the  feces  of  any  molt 
stage. 

Metabolite  profiles  for  stage  D2-D,  are  shown  in  Figure 
5.  The  major  hemolymph  ecdysteroid  at  this  stage  was 
20E,  with  smaller  amounts  of  HP1,  HP2,  20.26E,  and 
ecdysone.  The  urinary  profile  is  also  shown.  Fecal  [3H]- 
ecdysteroids  were  >99%  apolar  products.  Hydrolysis  of 
the  fecal  apolar  material  yielded  a  number  of  products 
(Fig.  6).  The  profiles  of  fecal  ecdysteroid  conjugates  varied 
according  to  molt  stage.  Figure  6  shows  the  profile  of  a 
premolt  stage  D,  lobster  fecal  sample  10  days  post-injec- 
tion; at  this  time,  20E  was  the  major  hemolymph  metab- 
olite. Hydrolysis  yielded  a  large  percentage  of  20E,  and 
smaller  amounts  of  HP1,  ecdysone,  P,  T,  and  unhydro- 
lyzed  apolar  components.  During  intermolt  stage  C,  hy- 
drolysis of  fecal  apolar  ecdysteroids  resulted  in  a  higher 
percentage  of  free  HP  (data  not  shown). 

The  uptake  and  metabolism  of  [3H]-ecdysone  injected 
into  hemolymph  was  studied  in  juvenile  lobsters  in  in- 
termolt stage  C  (Table  II).  By  1  h  after  injection,  only 
33%  of  the  radiolabel  remained  in  the  hemolymph.  Tissues 
such  as  hindgut,  antennal  glands,  immature  ovaries,  and 
epidermis  all  contained  <1%  of  the  injected  dose  at  1  h. 
Only  abdominal  muscle  (3.7%),  midgut  gland  (4.5%),  and 


1"  (HP1)  and  "highly  polar  2"  (HP2).  The  most  polar 
metabolite,  HP1,  was  a  mixture  of  conjugates  of  20EA, 
20.26E,  and  20E  and  also  contained  Helix  pomalia  en- 
zyme-resistant compounds.  The  urine  of  stage  D:  lobsters 
contained  significantly  more  HP1  material  as  enzyme- 
hydrolyzable  conjugates  than  any  other  molt  stage.  This 
result  indicates  that  ecdysteroid  conjugation  may  increase 
in  the  late  premolt  stage.  Hydrolysis  of  HP2  also  yielded 
20EA,  20.26E,  20E,  and  non-hydrolyzable  material. 
Raising  both  the  concentration  of  enzymes  and  incubation 
times  failed  to  increase  the  hydrolysis  of  HP1  and  HP2, 
supporting  the  idea  that  they  may  also  contain  other  types 
of  ecdysteroid  metabolites.  There  were  no  significant  dif- 
ferences between  any  of  the  molt  stages  in  20E  and  ec- 
dysone excretion  as  percentage  of  the  total  ecdysteroids 
excreted  per  day  in  urine.  But,  since  the  excretion  rates 
in  stages  C  and  D2  were  significantly  higher  in  days  1-6 
(Table  II)  than  those  in  Dn  and  D, ,  more  free  20E  and 
ecdysone  were  excreted  in  the  urine  of  the  former  stages. 
The  hemolymph  of  stage  C  lobsters,  96  h  after  injection, 
contained  mainly  HP2,  20EA,  20.26E,  and  20E.  Smaller 
quantities  of  HP  1 ,  ecdysone,  and  P  were  also  present.  The 
urine  of  stage  C  animals  contained  almost  90%.  HP  with 
HP2,  20.26E,  and  20E  in  similar  proportions.  The  feces 
contained  >95%>  apolar  material;  one  component  eluted 


30  -i 


*>          15 

Q. 

E 

o 
1/1  n 


Hemolymph 


O 

o 
<u 
rr 

o. 
u 


v 
0_ 


ion 


5 


40  i 


20- 


Urine 


Feces 


10 


20 


30 


40 


50 


60 


Retention  Time  (min) 

Figure  5.  Reverse  phase-HPLC-scintillation  spectrometric  analyses 
ofhemolymph,  urine,  and  feces  from  a  lobster  at  stage  D2-D3  (when  the 
hemolymph  ecdysteroids  reach  a  maximum).  The  animal  was  injected 
with  ['H]-ecdysone  in  stage  D,  and  sampled  96  h  later.  Separation  con- 
ditions are  described  in  Figure  1 .  Abbreviations  for  the  various  ecdysle- 
roids  are  as  in  Figure  4. 


480 


M.  J.  SNYDER  AND  E.  S.  CHANG 


carcass  (45%)  contained  appreciable  radioactivity  at  1  h. 
When  calculated  as  percentage  of  dose  per  gram  wet 
weight  of  tissue,  the  largest  amounts  were  found  (after  1 
h)  in  the  antennal  glands  (16%)  and  ovaries  (1 1%).  The 
smallest  quantities  on  a  per  weight  basis  were  found  (after 
1  h)  in  muscle  ( 1 .5%)  and  carcass  ( 1 .8%). 

By  10  days  post-injection,  about  41%>  of  the  injected 
dose  had  been  lost  due  to  excretion.  Concomitantly,  most 
tissues  had  very  low  levels  of  radioactivity;  hindgut,  an- 
tennal glands,  epidermis,  ovaries,  and  muscle  all  contained 
0.1-0.2%  of  the  initial  label.  Higher  levels  were  found  in 
hemolymph  (1.0%),  midgut  gland  (5.2%),  and  carcass 
(14%).  On  a  wet  weight  basis,  significantly  higher  levels 
were  associated  with  hindgut,  antennal  glands,  and  midgut 
gland  (30-32%).  Clearly,  by  10  days,  the  midgut  gland 
had  concentrated  much  more  ['H]-ecdysteroids  than  any 
other  single  tissue  examined. 

At  1  h  and  10  days,  1 1%  and  38%  of  the  injected  dose, 
respectively,  could  not  be  accounted  for  by  either  losses 
in  methanol  extraction  (81%  efficiency),  leakage  from  the 
injection  site  (about  2.5%,  as  judged  from  counts  of  ab- 
sorbent paper  held  on  the  wound  for  30  s  after  injection, 
and  from  counts  of  the  seawater  bath  1  h  later),  or  from 
adherence  to  the  injection  needle  (about  5%).  Possible 
explanations  for  these  unexplained  losses  are  [3H]  ex- 
change or  the  activity  of  side-chain  cleaving  enzymes;  the 
latter  has  been  suggested  to  occur  in  decapod  crustaceans 
(Lachaise  and  Lafont,  1984)  and  in  other  arthropods 
(Koolman  and  ivarlson,  1985). 

Some  tissue  extracts  from  1  hand  10  days  after  injection 
of  juvenile  lobsters  were  also  studied  by  RP-HPLC  (Fig. 


V 

o. 

E 
o 
in 


0> 
u 
V 
> 

o 

v 
(Z 

2 

Q. 
O 


30  n 


15- 


0 
30 


20E 


HP1 


-~A_ 


10 


20 


30 


40 


50 


60 


Retention  Time  (min) 


Figure  6.  Enzyme  treatment  of  fecal  ['H]-ecdysteroids  from  a  stage 
D,  lobster.  Ecdysteroids  were  isolated  from  feces  and  incubated  without 
(a)  or  with  (b)  Helix  /tuniutiu  sulfatase.  The  samples  were  then  analyzed 
by  reverse  phase-HPLC.  Separation  conditions  are  described  in  Figure 
1 .  Abbreviations  for  the  various  ecdysteroids  are  as  in  Figure  4. 


7).  In  1  h,  the  largest  amount  of  [3H]-ecdysteroids  in  he- 
molymph remained  as  ecdysone.  followed  by  smaller 
quantities  of  20E  and  P.  Only  about  2%.  of  the  dose  was 
found  as  HP  in  hemolymph  1  h  after  injection.  Abdominal 
muscle  contained  higher  levels  of  20E  than  hemolymph, 
but  ecdysone  was  still  the  major  ecdysteroid  at  1  h.  At  1 
h,  muscle  contained  slightly  higher  (about  5%)  levels  of 
HP  than  did  hemolymph.  Large  amounts  of  20.26E,  and 


Table  II 

Recovery  of  radioactivity  in  tissues  <>/  juvenile  lohstcrs  injected  will)  [3H]-ecdysone] 


Sample            n                H 

HG                      AG                    EP                   OV                      M                        MG                       C 

1  Ir                     4 

%of 

injected 

dose                   33.1  ±  3.0C 

C?    In 

0.2  ±    Oa              0.4±0.1h       0.9±0.lc         0.9  ±  0.2  c       3.7  ±  0.8"             4.5  ±  0.6d           45.4±6.1r 

™/g 
wet  wt.                   3.3  ±  1.0ac 

3.5  ±    0.8a-c       15.6±6.1h       3.8±1.0a       10.8  ±2.1"       1.5  ±  0.8c>d          2.8±0.9ac-d         1.8±0.2d 

10  days2             4 

%of 

injected 

dose                      l.0±0.2f 

0.2  ±    O.la           0.1±0"          0.1±0a            0.1±0"          0.4  ±  0"                5.2±l.ld            13.7  ±2.4' 

%/10g 

wet  wt.                   1.1  ±  0.2a 

30.3  +  25.4"         32.4+1.7d       4.0  ±1.3"        ll.7±7.8h       5.2±9.1ab'c       32.3  ±  5.  ld             6.0  +  0.4L 

1  Lobsters  were  injected  with  ['H]-ecdysone,  sacrificed  at  either  I  h  or  10  days;  methanolic  extracts  were  then  made  of  the  various  tissues:  (H 
=  hemolymph.  HG  =  hindgut,  AG  =  antennal  glands.  EP  =  epidermis  of  cephalothorax,  OV  =  ovaries.  M  =  abdominal  muscle,  MG  =  midgut 
gland,  and  C  =  remaining  carcass).  The  radioactivity  recovered  from  extracts  was  computed  from  determinations  on  aliquots  and  expressed  as  a 
percentage  of  the  total  injected  dose. 

2  The  data  are  presented,  either  as  a  percentage  of  the  injected  dose,  or  as  a  percentage  of  the  dose  per  gram  (for  1  h).  or  per  10  g  (for  10  days)  of 
wet  tissue.  Values  are  means  (±1  standard  deviation).  Different  superscript  letters  denote  values  that  are  significantly  different  (P  <  0.05)  from  each 
other  (within  a  row);  analysis  by  ANOVA  followed  by  Schefte  tests  of  the  arcsine  transformations  of  the  percentage  values  (Sokal  and  Rohlf,  1969). 


LOBSTER  ECDYSTEROID  METABOLISM 


481 


a 

E 
o 
(ft 
\ 
•o 
1) 


o 

o 


cr 


CL 
o 


80 
40 

0 
40 

20 


50 


25 


fM         '! 
rg  fl 

-  if 

a. 

i 


80 


40 


"c 
a> 
u 

Lkl 

M 
O 

e 

HI 

*     15 

LU 
ft, 

ni 

n 

li 

20 


0 


"0    10  20  30  40  50  60        010  20  30  40  50  60 
Retention  Time  (min) 

Figure  7.  Reverse  phase-HPLC  chromatograms  of  extracts  of  tissues 
from  juvenile  stage  C4  lobsters.  Hemolymph  (a,h),  abdominal  muscle 
(c.d).  and  midgut  gland  (e.f)  were  taken  at  either  I  h  (a.c.e)  or  10  days 
(b,d,f)  after  injection  with  ['H]-ecdysone.  Samples  were  separated 
using  gradient  #3.  Abbreviations  for  the  various  ecdysteroids  are  as  in 
Figure  4. 


approximately  equivalent  amounts  of  20E,  ecdysone,  and 
apolar  products  were  present  in  midgut  gland  at  1  h  after 
injection.  Smaller  amounts  of  HP2,  and  a  peak  eluting 
between  P  and  T,  were  also  found  in  the  midgut  gland  at 
1  h. 

By  10  days,  hemolymph  contained  mostly  HP  with  a 
large  peak  of  20,26E  and  smaller  quantities  of  HP1,  20EA, 
HP2,  and  20E.  In  contrast,  a  large  peak  of  HP1  and  very 
small  amounts  of  20EA,  20.26E,  and  20E  were  present 
in  the  muscle  at  10  days.  A  large  amount  of  apolar  material 
and  smaller  quantities  of  HP1,  20EA,  20.26E,  20E.  and 
ecdysone  were  found  in  the  midgut  gland  at  10  days. 

Discussion 

At  all  molt  stages,  ecdysone  was  very  rapidly  eliminated 
from  lobster  hemolymph.  The  rapid  loss  of  [3H]-ecdysone 
was  not  unexpected,  because  ecdysone  tilers  in  H.  amer- 
icanm  (as  determined  by  RIA)  were  never  more  than  19% 
of  the  total  ecdysteroids  in  hemolymph  (Snyder  and 


Chang,  199 la).  Ecdysone  also  has  a  short  half-life  in  the 
crab  Gecarcinus  lalcralis  (McCarthy,  1980,  1982)  and  in 
several  insect  species  (reviewed  by  Koolman  and  Karlson, 
1985;  Koolman  and  Walter,  1985).  The  activity  of  ec- 
dysone 20-monooxygenase,  which  converts  ecdysone  to 
20-hydroxyecdysone  (20E),  probably  has  a  role  in  chang- 
ing the  levels  of  ecdysone  in  lobsters,  as  it  does  in  several 
insects  (Smith  et  ai.  1983:  Mitchell  and  Smith,  1988). 
Young  (1976)  treated  blowflies  with  supraphysiological 
doses  of  ecdysone,  and  demonstrated  that  alterations  in 
the  conversion  rates  of  ecdysone  to  20E  could  not  be  ex- 
plained by  saturation  of  metabolizing  sites.  Lachaise  et 
ai  ( 1 976)  reported  that  the  rate  of  conversion  of  ecdysone 
to  20E  was  slower  in  postmolt  and  intermolt  than  in  pre- 
molt  stage  crabs.  SoumofF  and  Skinner  (1988)  demon- 
strated that  enzyme  activity  varied  with  molt  cycle  in  G. 
lateralis  and  that  the  variations  were  lowest  in  late  premolt 
and  postmolt.  Additionally,  Chang  and  O'Connor  (1978) 
showed  that  20-hydroxylation  activity  increased  by  four 
times  in  the  testes  of  crabs  (Pachygrapsus  crassipes)  that 
had  undergone  eyestalk  ablation.  The  variation  in  ecdy- 
sone 20-monooxygenase  during  the  crustacean  molt  cycle 
is  still  not  understood. 

Coincident  with  the  drop  in  ['H]-ecdysone  were  in- 
creases in  [3H]-20E.  The  primary  metabolite  of  ecdysone 
in  other  crustaceans  has  been  shown  to  be  20E  (Lachaise 
et  ai.  1976;  Chang  and  O'Connor.  1978;  Kuppert  et  ai, 
1978;  McCarthy,  1980,  1982;  Buchholz,  1982).  The  rel- 
ative amounts  of  [3H]-20E,  following  the  peak  of  conver- 
sion from  [3H]-ecdysone  at  4-12  h,  were  significantly 
higher  in  the  premolt  stages  of  lobsters.  In  the  late  premolt 
stage  D:.  there  was  a  rapid  loss  of  [3H]-20E  after  24  h 
similar  to  that  exhibited  in  stages  A  and  C  after  4  h.  These 
results  are  suggestive  of  a  mechanism  that  regulates  ec- 
dysteroid  metabolism  around  the  time  of  the  late  premolt 
peak  in  the  hemolymph  (Snyder  and  Chang,  199 la). 
McCarthy  (1982)  reported  long  hemolymph  20E  half-lives 
for  G.  lateralis  in  early-mid  premolt.  Long  half-lives  for 
20E  in  early  premolt  crabs  were  significantly  reduced  by 
the  autotomy  of  partially  regenerated  limbs,  suggesting 
that  other  controls  of  ecdysteroid  metabolism  exist 
(McCarthy,  1980).  Others  have  reported  that  20E  catab- 
olism  in  insects  can  vary  over  larval-pupal  stages  (reviewed 
by  Lehmann  and  Koolman,  1989).  As  in  the  lobster  molt 
cycle,  20E  was  lost  at  a  faster  rate  when  molting  hormone 
tilers  were  increasing  in  the  blowfly  Calliphora  vicina 
(Young,  1976;  Young  and  Young,  1976;  Koolman  and 
Walter,  1985)  and  in  the  tobacco  hornworm  Manduca 
sexta  (reviewed  by  Gilbert,  1989).  The  potential  roles  of 
20E  catabolic  aclivity  in  the  regulation  of  crustacean  ec- 
dysleroid  lilers  require  further  study. 

Levels  of  highly  polar  ['H]-ecdysteroid  metabolites 
(HP),  such  as  20-hydroxyecdysonoic  acid  (20EA),  20,26- 
dihydroxyecdysone  (20.26E),  and  conjugates,  increased 


482 


M.  J.  SNYDER  AND  E.  S.  CHANG 


in  hemolymph  after  increases  in  20E.  In  H.  americainis, 
hemolymph  metabolites  appeared  in  the  following  order: 
ecdysone  -*•  20E  -*•  HP.  The  metabolism  of  a  single  in- 
jected dose  of  [3H]-ecdysone  appeared  to  mimic  normal 
ecdysteroid  metabolism  in  lobsters.  Highly  polar  metab- 
olites were  the  major  circulating  ecdysteroids  in  all  lobster 
molt  stages,  except  during  mid-late  premolt,  the  period 
when  the  major  peak  of  ecdysteroids  occurs  in  the  he- 
molymph (Snyder  and  Chang,  199 la).  However,  of  the 
metabolites  detected  by  RIA  in  lobster  hemolymph.  urine, 
and  feces  (Snyder  and  Chang,  1991a,b),  not  all  were  found 
after  the  injection  of  pHj-ecdysone.  A  few  unidentified 
metabolites  eluting  from  the  RP  column  between  ecdy- 
sone and  P  were  absent  in  the  present  study.  Gilbert  (1989) 
advised  caution  in  the  interpretation  of  [3H]-ecdysone  in- 
jection experiments,  as  similar  incomplete  ecdysteroid 
profiles  were  found  in  Manduca  sexta. 

Injections  of  [3H]-ecdysone  into  cannulated  lobsters 
confirmed  that  the  likely  route  for  excretion  of  ecdysteroid 
metabolites  (HP,  and  unconjugated  20E,  ecdysone,  and 
P)  from  hemolymph  is  via  the  urine  in  all  molt  stages. 
Equivalent  results  were  found  when  excreta  were  assayed 
by  RIA  throughout  the  molt  cycle  (Snyder  and  Chang, 
1 99 1  b).  Others  have  shown  that  HP  were  formed  by  deca- 
pod crustaceans  following  the  injection  of  pH]-ecdysone 
or  ['H]-P  (Lachaise  el  ai.  1976;  Kuppert  el  ai.  1978; 
McCarthy,  1980.  1982;  Buchholz,  1982;  Lachaise  and 
Lafont,  1984).  Some  of  these  metabolites,  including  con- 
jugates, have  also  been  found  in  the  seawater  surrounding 
the  animals  (Buchholz,  1982;  Lachaise  and  Lafont,  1984). 
The  only  differences  related  to  the  molt  cycle  that  were 
discovered  in  lobster  urine  were  in  the  amounts  of  HP 
conjugates,  which  were  much  higher  in  late  premolt  (stage 
Di)  lobsters.  It  may  be  that  highly  polar  conjugates  are 
destined  for  excretion  only  in  non-reproductive  lobsters, 
and  that  excretion  is  more  significant  during  the  rapid 
decline  in  ecdysteroid  titer  just  prior  to  ecdysis.  Lachaise 
and  Lafont  ( 1984)  found  similar  increases  in  highly  polar 
ecdysteroid  conjugates  in  late  premolt  crabs  (Carcinus 
maenas)  after  injection  of  ponasterone  A.  Polar  conjugates 
are  loaded  into  vitellogenic  ovaries,  and  thus  may  be  po- 
tential sources  of  ecdysteroids  for  developing  crustacean 
embryos  (Lachaise  el  ai.  1981;  Spindler  el  ai.  1987). 

The  data  on  excretion  rates  (Table  I),  suggest  that  lob- 
sters have  an  additional  mechanism  for  regulating  ecdy- 
steroid levels.  Initial  excretion  rates  were  higher  for  in- 
termolt  (stage  C)  and  late  premolt  (stage  D2)  lobsters  after 
the  final  hemolymph  peak.  Additionally,  the  excretion 
rate  increased  in  mid-premolt  (stage  D, ),  in  the  latter  part 
of  the  1 5-day  observation  period,  at  a  time  after  injection 
equivalent  to  stage  D:  lobsters.  The  data  indicate  that 
excretion  rates  vary  with  the  stage  of  the  molt  cycle;  reg- 
ulation of  these  excretion  rates  may  therefore  be  an  ad- 
ditional means  of  altering  ecdysteroid  liters.  These  results 


agree  with  earlier  studies  on  insects.  Hoffmann  et  ai 
(1974)  and  Koolman  and  Walter  ( 1985)  provided  evidence 
that  excretion  rates  varied  and  were  lowest  at  times  of 
peak  hormone  titer  in  locusts  and  blowflies.  The  role  of 
excretion  in  regulating  ecdysteroid  liters  in  crustaceans 
remains  obscure. 

Excretion  of  ['H]-ecdysteroids  in  lobster  feces  was  de- 
tecled  following  injeclion  of  ['H]-ecdysone  inlo  hemo- 
lymph. These  data  confirm  those  from  earlier  RIA  data 
(Snyder  and  Chang,  1991b),  indicaling  lhal  the  lobster 
gut  can  absorb  ecdysleroids  from  hemolymph  and  Irans- 
form  Ihem  inlo  apolar  conjugales  prior  lo  Iheir  excrelion 
in  feces.  Apolar  ecdysteroid  conjugates  were  also  found 
in  larval  crabs  after  ihe  injeclion  of  [3H]-ecdysone  (Connal 
and  Diehl,  1986).  The  apolar  malerial  in  lobsler  feces 
consisled  of  conjugales  of  HP  melaboliles  and  20,26E, 
20E,  ecdysone.  and  P.  Apolar  ecdysleroid-conjugaling 
enzymes  in  Ihe  gul  are.  Iherefore,  nol  specific  for  particular 
melaboliles.  Conjugaling  enzymes  are  similarly  non-spe- 
cific in  spiders  (Connal  et  ai,  1988c),  licks  (Connal  et  ai, 
1988b),  mealworms  (Delbecquerttf/.,  1988),  and  crickels 
(Whiling  and  Dinan,  1988).  The  apolar  melaboliles  have 
been  identified  as  long-chain  fally  acid  eslers  in  a  variely 
of  arthropods,  but  their  definilive  idenlification  in  crus- 
taceans awails  further  sludy  (Hoffman  et  ai.  1985:  Kubo 
et  ai.  1987;  Whiling  and  Dinan,  1989).  The  failure  of 
olhers  lo  find  apolar  conjugales  as  major  ecdysleroid  me- 
tabolites  of  arthropods  has  been  attributed  to  losses  in 
purificalion  or  in  ihe  choice  of  HPLC  condilions  (Connat 
and  Diehl.  1986). 

When  ingested  by  lobslers,  [3H]-ecdysone  is  converted 
lo  apolar  conjugates  withoul  further  melabolism  lo  olher 
ecdysleroids  or  absorplion  from  Ihe  gut  (Snyder  and 
Chang,  1991b).  Similarly,  ingested  ecdysteroids  are  effi- 
cienlly  "detoxified"  to  apolar  metabolites  in  a  variety  of 
arthropods  including  spiders  (Connat  et  ai.  1988c),  licks 
(Connal  et  ai,  1988a),  and  tobacco  budworms  (Kubo  et 
ai,  1987).  The  role  of  the  lobsler  midgul  gland  in  apolar 
ecdysteroid  conjugalion  was  confirmed  by  injeclion  of 
['H]-ecdysone  (Table  II;  Fig.  7).  Appreciable  amounls  of 
apolar  conjugales  were  only  found  in  Ihe  midgul  gland. 
This  finding  parallels  resulls  derived  from  in  vitro  sludies 
of  Ihe  lobster  (Snyder  and  Chang,  1992)  and  crayfish  mid- 
gul glands  (Gorell  et  ai,  1972).  Appreciable  amounls  of 
apolar  conjugates  were  also  found  in  the  crayfish  midgut 
gland  after  injection  of  [3H]-ecdysone  into  Ihe  hemolymph 
(Kuppert  et  ai.  1978).  The  funclion  of  Ihe  midgul  gland 
is  slill  unclear  in  relalion  lo  ils  role  in  ecdysleroid  melab- 
olism; il  mighl  be  Ihe  slow  release  of  apolar  conjugates 
inlo  Ihe  feces,  or  Ihe  provision,  after  hydrolysis,  of  an 
addilional  source  of  aclive  hormone.  Bolh  Sehnal  et  ai 
(1981)  and  Williams  (1987)  have  shown  thai  Ihe  insecl 
pupal  midgul  conlains  a  mobilizable  source  of  ecdysle- 
roids that  is  sufficient  to  drive  the  pupal-adult  transition 


LOBSTER  ECDYSTEROID  METABOLISM 


483 


in  the  absence  of  prothoracic  glands.  The  lack  of  appre- 
ciable absorption  of  [3H]-ecdysone  after  ingestion  by  H. 
americanus  argues  that  the  sole  function  of  this  ecdyste- 
roid  metabolic  route  in  crustaceans  may  be  for  excretion. 

Apolar  ecdysteroid  conjugates  have  been  found  in  ova- 
ries and  embryos  in  other  arthropods,  such  as  ticks  (Con- 
nat  ct  al.,  1988b),  cockroaches  (Slinger  and  Isaac,  1988), 
and  crickets  (Whiting  and  Dinan,  1988,  1989).  Similar 
studies  should  be  conducted  on  crustaceans  to  determine 
the  presence  of  these  apolar  conjugates  in  ovaries  and 
embryos. 

In  conclusion,  the  metabolism  of  [3H]-ecdysteroids  in 
H.  americanus  involves  both  polar  and  apolar  pathways. 
The  overall  metabolic  routes  of  lobster  ecdysteroids  are 
therefore  similar  to  those  found  in  a  variety  of  other  ar- 
thropods. 

Acknowledgments 

We  gratefully  acknowledge  the  gifts  of  ponasterone  A 
from  Dr.  J.  D.  O'Connor  (University  of  North  Carolina, 
Chapel  Hill),  and  20,26-dihydroxyecdysone  and  20-hy- 
droxyecdysonoic  acid  from  Dr.  M.  J.  Thompson  (U.  S. 
Department  of  Agriculture,  Beltsville,  Maryland).  We  also 
thank  Drs.  B.  L.  Lasley  and  J.-H.  Cheng  for  helpful  dis- 
cussions and  the  Editors  and  anonymous  reviewers  whose 
suggestions  improved  this  paper.  This  work  was  supported 
by  the  NOAA,  National  Sea  Grant  College  Program,  De- 
partment of  Commerce,  under  Grant  NA85AA-D-SG140, 
Project  R/A-80,  through  the  California  Sea  Grant  College 
Program  (to  E.S.C.).  The  U.  S.  Government  is  authorized 
to  reproduce  and  distribute  copies  for  governmental  pur- 
poses. 

Literature  Cited 

\ikrn.  D.  K.  1973.     Proecdysis,  setal  development,  and  molt  prediction 

in  the  American  lobster  (Homanis  americanus).  J  Fish.  Res  Board 

Can  30:  1337-1344. 
Buchhol;,  K.  1982.     The  metabolism  of  ecdysone  and  its  putative  role 

as  the  female  sex-pheromone  in  the  green  shore  crab  Carcimis  maenas 

L.  Puhl.  Cent.  Nail.  Exploit.  Oceans  Acles  Colloq.  14:  35-46. 
Chang,  K.  S.  1989.     Endocrine  regulation  of  molting  in  Crustacea.  Rev. 

AiinaticSci.  1:  131-157. 
Chang,  E.  S.,  and  D.  E.  Conklin.  1983.     Lobster  (Homanis)  hatchery 

techniques.  Pp.  271-275  in  CRC Handbook  of  Mariculture,  Vol.  1, 

J.  P.  McVey,  ed.  CRC  Press,  Boca  Raton. 
Chang,  E.  S.,  and  J.  D.  O'Connor.  1977.     Secretion  of  «-ecdysone  by 

crab  Y-organs  in  vitro.  Proc.  Nail.  Acad.  Sci.  USA  74:  615-618. 
Chang,  E.  S.,  and  J.  D.  O'Connor.  1978.     In  vitro  secretion  and  hy- 

droxylation  of  a-ecdysone  as  a  function  of  the  crustacean  molt  cycle. 

Gen.  Comp  Endocrinol:  36:  151-160. 
Christ,  B.,  and  D.  Sedlmeier.  1987.     Variations  in  epidermal  cyclic  nu- 

cleotide-dependent  protein  kinase  activity  during  moult  cycle  of  the 

crayfish  Orconectes  limosus  and  hormonal  control  of  kinase  activity 

by  20-hydroxyecdysone.  Int.  ./.  Biochem.  19:  79-84. 
Conklin,  D.  E.,  and  E.  S.  Chang.  1983.     Grow-out  techniques  for  the 

American   lobster,   Homanis  americanus.   Pp.   277-286   in  CRC 


Handbook  of  Manculturc.  Vol.  1,  J.  P.  McVey,  ed.  CRC  Press.  Boca 
Raton. 

Connat,  J.  L.,  and  P.  A.  Diehl.  1986.  Probable  occurrence  of  ecdysteroid 
fatty  acid  esters  in  different  classes  of  arthropods.  Insect  Biochem 
16:91-97. 

Connat,  J.  L.,  E.  M.  Dotson,  and  P.  A.  Diehl.  1988a.  Apolar  conjugates 
of  ecdysteroids  are  not  used  as  a  storage  form  of  molting  hormone 
in  the  argasid  tick  Ornithodoros  moubata.  Arch.  Insect  Biochem 
Physiol.  9:221-235. 

Connat,  J.  L.,  E.  M.  Dotson,  and  P.  A.  Diehl.  I988b.  Metabolism  of 
ecdysteroids  in  the  female  tick  Amblyomma  hebraeum  (Ixodoidea, 
Ixodidae):  accumulation  of  free  ecdysone  and  20-hydroxyecdysone 
in  the  eggs.  /  Com/'.  Pliysiol  B  157:  689-699. 

Connat,  J.  L.,  P.  A.  Fiirst,  and  M.  Zweilin.  1988c.  Detoxification  of 
injected  and  ingested  ecdysteroids  in  spiders.  Comp.  Biochem.  Physiol. 
91B:  257-265. 

Delbecque,  J.  P.,  J.  L.  Connat,  and  R.  Lafont.  1988.  Polar  and  apolar 
metabolites  of  ecdysteroids  during  the  metamorphosis  of  Tenebrio 
molitor.  J  Insect  Physiol  34:  619-624. 

Gilbert,  L.  I.  1989.  The  endocrine  control  of  molting:  the  tobacco 
hornworm,  Mandiica  sexta.  as  a  model  system.  Pp.  448-47 1  in  Ec- 
dysone: From  Chemistry  to  Mode  of  Action.  J.  Koolman.  ed.  Georg 
Thieme  Verlag.  Stuttgart. 

Gorell,  T.  A.,  L.  1.  Gilbert,  and  J.  B.  Siddall.  1972.     Binding  proteins 
for  an  ecdysone  metabolite  in  the  crustacean  hepatopancreas.  Proc. 
Nail.  Acad.  Sci.  USA  69:  812-815. 
Hampshire,  F.,  and  D.  H.  S.  Horn.  1966.     Structure  of  crustecdysone. 

a  crustacean  molting  hormone.  Chem.  Commun.  2:  37-38. 
Hoffmann,  J.  A.,  J.  Koolman,  P.  Karlson,  and  P.  Joly.  1974.     Molting 
hormone  liter  and  metabolic  fate  of  injected  ecdysone  during  the 
fifth  larval  instar  and  in  adults  of  Locusta  migratona  (Orthoptera). 
Gen  Comp.  Endocrinol  22:  90-97. 

Hoffmann,  K.  H.,  D.  Bulenda,  E.  Thiry,  and  E.  Schmid.  1985.     Apolar 
ecdysteroid  esters  in  adult  female  crickets,  Gryllus  himacn/atiis.  Life 
Sci.  37:  185-192. 
Holliday,  C.  W.  1977.     A  new  method  for  measuring  the  urinary  rate 

of  a  brachyuran  crab.  Comp  Biochem.  Physiol.  58A:  1 19-120. 
Hopkins,  P.  M.  1986.     Ecdysteroid  tilers  and  Y-organ  activity  during 
late  anecdysis  and  proecdysis  in  the  fiddler  crab,  Uca  pugilator.  Gen. 
Comp.  Endocrinol  63:  362-373. 

Isaac,  R.  E.,  and  A.  J.  Slinger.  1989.  Storage  and  excrelion  of  ecdy- 
steroids. Pp.  250-253  in  Ecdysone:  From  Chemistry  to  Mode  of  Ac- 
tion. J.  Roolman,  ed.  Georg  Thieme  Verlag,  Stuttgart. 
Koolman,  J.,  and  P.  Karlson.  1985.  Regulation  of  ecdysteroid  tiler: 
degradalion.  Pp.  343-361  in  Comprehensive  Insect  Physiology  Bio- 
chemistry and  Pharmacology.  Vol.  7,  G.  A.  Kerkul  and  L.  I.  Gilbert, 
eds.  Pergamon  Press,  Oxford. 

Koolman,  J.,  and  J.  Walter.  1985.  Ecdysleroids  in  insects:  how  are 
Iheir  concenlralions  regulaled?  Pp.  1 98-220  in  Metamorphosis:  Brit- 
ish Society  for  Developmental  Biology  Symposium.  Vol.  8.  M.  Balls 
and  M.  Bownes.  eds.  Clarendon  Press,  Oxford. 

Kubo,  I.,  S.  Komatsu,  Y.  Asaka,  and  G.  De  Boer.  1987.     Isolalion  and 
identification  of  apolar  metabolites  of  ingested  20-hydroxyecdysone 
in  trass  of  He/iothis  virescens  larvae.  J  Chem.  Ecol.  13:  785-794. 
Kuppert,  P.,  M.  Biichler,  and  K.-D.  Spindler.  1978.     Distribulion  and 
transport  of  molting  hormones  in  the  crayfish,  Orconectes  limosus. 
Z  Naturforsch.  33C:  437-441. 
Lachaise,  F.,  and  R.  Lafont.  1984.     Ecdysteroid  metabolism  in  a  crab: 

Carcinus  maenas  L.  Steroids  43:  243-259. 

Lachaise,  F.,  M.  Lagueuv,  R.  Feyereisen,  and  J.  A.  Hoffmann. 
1976.  Metabolisme  de  1'ecdysone  au  cours  du  developpement  de 
Carcinus  maenas  ( Brachyura.  Decapoda).  C.  R  Acad.  Sci.  Paris  283: 
943-946. 


484 


M.  J.  SNYDER  AND  E.  S.  CHANG 


Lachaise,  F.,  M.  Goudeau,  C.  Hetru.  C.  Kappler,  and  J.  A.  Hoffmann. 

1981.     Ecdysleroids  and  ovarian  development  in  the  shore  crab. 

( \IICIHUS  inaenas.  Hoppe-Seyler's  /.  Physiol.  (.'hem  363:  1059-1067. 
I.ai-haisr,  F.,  G.  Carpenter,  G.  Somme,  J.  Colandeau,  and  P.  Beydon. 

1989.     Ecdysteroid  synthesis  by  crab  Y-organs.  J  Exp.  Zool,  252: 

283-292. 

Lehmann.  M..  and  J.  Koulman.  1989.     Regulation  of  ecdysone  metab- 
olism. Pp.  2 1 7-220  in  Ecdysone:  From  Chemistry  to  Mode  oj  Action, 

J.  Koolman,  ed.  Georg  Thieme  Verlag.  Stuttgart. 
McCarthy,  J.  F.  1980.     Ecdysone  metabolism  and  the  interruption  of 

proecdysis  in  the  land  crab,  Gecarcinus  lateralis  Biol.  Bull.  158: 91- 

102. 
McCarthy,  J.  F.  1982.     Ecdysone  metabolism  in  premolt  land  crabs 

(Gecarcinus  lateralis).  Gen.  Camp.  Endocrinol.  47:  323-332. 
Mitchell,  M.  J.,  and  S.  L.  Smith.  1988.     Ecdysone  20-monooxygenase 

activity  throughout  the  life  cycle  of  Drosophila  melanogaster.  Gen. 

Comp.  Endocrinol.  72:  467-470. 
Mykles,  D.  L.  1980.     The  mechanism  of  fluid  absorption  at  ecdysis  in 

the  American  lobster,  Homarus  amencanus.  J.  Exp.  Biol.  84:  89- 

101. 
Sehnal,  F.,  P.  Maroy,  and  J.  Mala.  1981.     Regulation  and  significance 

of  ecdysteroid  litre  fluctuations  in  lepidopterous  larvae  and  pupae. 

./.  Insect  Physiol.  27:  535-544. 
Slinger,  A.  J.,  and  R.  E.  Isaac.  1988.     Synthesis  of  apolar  ecdysone 

esters  by  ovaries  of  the  cockroach  Periplaneta  americana.  Gen.  Comp. 

Endocrinol  70:  74-82. 
Smith,  S.  L.,  W.  E.  Bollenbacher,  and  L.  I.  Gilbert.  1983.     Ecdysone 

20-monooxygenase   activity   during  larval-pupal   development   of 

Manduca  sex/a.  Mol.  Cell.  Endocr.  31:  227-251. 
Snyder,  M.  J.,  and  E.  S.  Chang.  1991a.     Ecdysteroids  in  relation  to  the 

molt  cycle  of  the  American  lobster.  Homarus  americanus    I.  He- 

molymph  liters  and  metabolites.  Gen.  Comp.  Endocrinol.  81:  133- 

145. 
Snyder,  M.  J.,  and  E.  S.  Chang.  1991b.     Ecdysteroids  in  relation  to  the 

molt  cycle  of  the  American  lobster,  Homarus  amencanus.  II.  Excre- 
tion of  metabolites.  Gen.  Comp.  Endocrinol.  (in  press). 


Snyder,  M.  J.,  and  E.  S.  Chang.  1992.     Role  of  the  midgut  gland  in  the 

metabolism  and  excretion  of  ecdysteroids  by  lobsters.  Homarus 

americanus  Gen  Comp  Endocrinol.  (in  press). 
Sokal,  R.  R.,  and  F.  J.  Rohlf.  1969.     Biometry.  W.  H.  Freeman.  San 

Francisco.  776  pp. 
Soumoff,  C.,  and  D.  M.  Skinner.  1988.     Ecdysone  20-monooxygenase 

activity  in  land  crabs.  Comp.  Biochem.  Physiol.  91C:  139-144. 
Spaziani,    E.,    H.    H.    Rees,    W.    L.    Wang,    and    R.    D.    Watson. 

1989.     Evidence  that  Y-organs  of  the  crab  Cancer  antennarius  secrete 

3-dehydroecdysone.  Mol  Cell.  Endocr  66:  17-25. 
Spindler,  K.-D.,  A.  Van  Wormhoudt,  D.  Sellos,  and  M.  Spindler-Barth. 

1987.     Ecdysteroid  levels  during  embryogenesis  in  the  shrimp,  Pa- 

laemon  serratus  (Crustacea  Decapoda):  quantitative  and  qualitative 

changes.  Gen.  Comp.  Endocrinol.  66:  1  16-122. 
Stevenson,  J.  R.  1968.     Metecdysial  molt  staging  and  changes  in  the 

cuticle  in  the  crayfish  Orconectes  sanborni  (Faxon).  Crustaceana  14: 

169-177. 
Traub,  M.,  G.  Gellissen,  and  K.-D.  Spindler.  1987.     20(OH)ecdysone- 

induced  transition  from  intermolt  to  premolt  protein  biosynthesis 

patterns  in  the  hypodermis  of  the  crayfish.  Astacus  leptodactylus.  in 

vitro.  Gen.  Comp.  Endocrinol.  65:  465-477. 
Whiting,  P.,  and  I,.  Dinan.  1988.     The  occurrence  of  apolar  ecdysteroid 

conjugates  in  newly-laid  eggs  of  the  house  cricket,  Acheta  domesticus. 

J  Insect  Physiol.  34:  625-63 1 . 
Whiting,  P.,  and  I,.  Dinan.  1989.     Identification  of  the  endogenous  apolar 

ecdysteroid  conjugates  present  in  newly-laid  eggs  of  the  house  cricket 

(Acheta  domesticus)  as  22-long-chain  fatty  acyl  esters  of  ecdysone. 

Insect  Biochem.  19:  759-765. 
Williams,  C.  M.  1987.     Midgut  oflepidopteran  pupae  is  a  major  depot 

of  sequestered,  mobilizable,  ecdysteroids.  Memor.  Instil.  Oswaldo 

Cm:  82:  47-49. 
Young,  !N.  I,.  1976.     The  metabolism  of  3H-molting  hormone  in  Cal- 

liphora  erythrocephala  at  the  mature  larval  and  white  puparial  stages. 

Insect  Biochem.  6:  1-12. 
Young,  N.  I,.,  and  P.  R.  Young.  1976.     Biochemical  control  of  moulting 

hormone  liter  in  Calliphora  erythrocephala  during  puparium  for- 
mation. Insect  Biochem  6:  169-177. 


Reference:  Biol  Bull.  180:  485-488.  (June,  1991) 


New  Calcitonin  Isolated  from  the  Ray,  Dasyatis  akajei 


Y.  TAKEI1*,   A.  TAKAHASHI2,  T.  X.  WATANABE3,  K.  NAKAJIMA1,  S.  SAKAKIBARA', 
Y.  SASAYAMA4.  N.  SUZUKI4.  AND  C.  OGURO4 

Departments  of ]  Physiology  and 2 Molecular  Biology,  Kitasato  University  Se/iool  of  Medicine. 

Sagamihara.  Kanagawa  228.  ^Peptide  Institute  Inc.,  Protein  Research  Foundation,  Minoh,  Osaka 

562,  and*  Department  of  Biology,  Faculty  of  Science.  Toyanui  University,  Toyama  930,  Japan 


Abstract.  Calcitonin  causes  hypocalcemia  by  inhibiting 
the  resorption  of  calcium  from  the  bone  in  mammals. 
Calcitonin  has  now  been  isolated  from  the  ultimobran- 
chial  gland  of  a  cartilaginous  fish,  the  ray  (Dasyatis  akajei), 
and  its  amino  acid  has  been  determined  to  be  H-Cys-Thr- 
Ser-Leu-Ser-Thr-Cys-Val-Val-Gly-Lys-Ser-Gln-Gln-Leu- 
His-Lys-Leu-Gln-Asn-Ile-Gln-Arg-Thr-Asp-Val-Gly-Ala- 
Ala-Thr-Pro-NHi.  Although  its  basic  structure  is  well 
conserved,  the  amino  acid  sequence  of  ray  calcitonin  is 
considerably  different  from  that  of  other  calcitonins  se- 
quenced  to  date.  Because  the  ray  lacks  calcified  bones,  an 
examination  of  the  effect  of  calcitonin  in  this  fish  may 
elucidate  a  new  role  for  calcitonin  in  vertebrates. 

Introduction 

Calcitonins  were  first  isolated  from  the  thyroid  glands 
of  mammals,  and  amino  acid  sequences  have  now  been 
determined  in  five  species  (Neher  et  al.,  1968;  Potts  et  al., 
1968,  1971;  Brewer  and  Ronan,  1969;  Raulais  et  al.. 
1976).  Although  calcitonin-like  immunoreactivity  was 
also  identified  in  the  ultimobranchial  glands  of  all  classes 
of  non-mammalian  vertebrates  (Van  Noorden  and  Pearse, 
1971;  Tisserand-Jochem  et  al..  1977;  Sasayama  et  at., 
1984;  Treilhou-Lahille  et  al.,  1984),  structures  have  been 
determined  for  only  one  species  of  bird  and  two  teleost 
fishes  (Niallrta/..  1969;NodaandNarita,  1976;Homma 
et  al..  1986).  The  mammalian  calcitonins  fall  into  two 
groups  according  to  the  homology  of  their  amino  acid 
sequences,  and  the  difference  between  the  amino  acid 
sequences  of  these  two  mammalian  groups  is  greater  than 
that  between  the  bird  and  the  teleosts  (Fig.  1 ).  As  for  func- 
tion, non-mammalian  calcitonins  have  a  much  greater 


Received  22  February  1991;  accepted  11  March  1991. 
*  To  whom  correspondence  should  be  sent. 


hypocalcemic  effect  in  the  rat  than  do  the  mammalian 
calcitonins  (Homma  et  al..  1986). 

Recently,  immunoreactive  calcitonin  was  demonstrated 
in  the  ray,  Dasyatis  akajei  (Sasayama  et  al..  1984).  Because 
the  osteocytes  are  the  principal  site  of  action  of  calcitonin 
in  mammals  (Friedman  and  Raisz,  1965),  a  new  action 
of  calcitonin  should  be  expected  in  this  cartilaginous  fish. 
Indeed,  mammalian  calcitonin  has  been  shown  to  lack  a 
hypocalcemic  effect  in  some  non-mammalian  species 
(Pang  et  al..  1980).  Thus,  the  roles  of  calcitonin  in  the 
cartilaginous  fish  may  provide  a  new  insight  into  the  fun- 
damental actions  of  calcitonin  common  to  all  vertebrates. 
As  the  essential  step  toward  discovering  such  roles,  an 
attempt  was  made  to  isolate  calcitonin  from  the  ray  and 
to  determine  its  amino  acid  sequence.  The  molecular 
structure  of  calcitonin  in  this  phylogenetically  primitive 
fish  may  provide  new  evidence  for  the  evolution  of  the 
calcitonin  molecule  in  vertebrate  phylogeny. 

Materials  and  Methods 

The  rays  (Dasyatis  akajei)  were  caught  in  Toyama  Bay 
and  anesthetized  with  1/3,000  (v/v)  of  tricaine  methane- 
sulfonate  (Sigma)  in  seawater.  The  ultimobranchial  glands 
were  resected  under  a  dissecting  microscope,  immediately 
frozen,  and  kept  at  — 50°C  until  used. 

Calcitonin  was  extracted  from  ray  ultimobranchial 
glands  and  purified  as  follows.  Two  hundred  deep-frozen 
glands  (2.2  g)  were  pulverized  in  a  stainless-steel  crusher 
with  a  hammer,  immediately  boiled  for  5  min  with  7  vol- 
umes of  water,  acidified  with  acetic  acid  to  make  a  final 
concentration  of  1  A/,  and  homogenized  in  a  Polytron 
homogenizer  for  90  s  at  4°C  at  maximum  speed  (Takei 
et  al..  1989).  The  homogenate  was  centrifuged  at  25,000 
X  g  for  30  min  at  4°C,  and  the  high  molecular  weight 
proteins  and  lipids  were  removed  from  the  supernatant 


485 


486 


Y.  TAKEI  ET  AL. 


C|AS  LSTCVLGKLSQELHKLQTYPRT  DV 
N  LSTCVLGKLSQELHKLQTYPRT  DV 
•N  LSTCVLGKLSQELHKLQTYPRT  NT 


A|GTP 


;TF 


-NH2 
-NH2 


-NHj 


CGNLSTCMLGTYTQD 
CGNLSTCMLGTYTQD 

: 

F 

NKFHTFPQT 
NKFHTFPQT 

S 

•-. 

IGVGAP 
IGVGAP 

-NH2 
-NH2 

Fowl    CT 
Eel   CT 
Salmon   CT 

Rat    CT 
Human   CT 

Porcine  CT 
Bovine  CT 
Ovine  CT 


Figure  1.  Amino  acid  sequences  of  the  mammalian  and  non-mam- 
malian calcitonins  (CTs)  that  have  been  sequenced  to  date.  The  identical 
amino  acids  within  the  same  group  are  boxed. 


with  67%  and  98.5%  acetone,  respectively,  at  4°C.  The 
extract  was  then  subjected  to  reverse-phase  high  perfor- 
mance liquid  chromatography  (HPLC)  on  an  ODS-120T 
column  (4.6  X  250  mm;  Tosoh,  Tokyo)  with  a  linear  gra- 
dient elution  from  20%  to  80%  CH,CN  in  0.1%  trifluo- 
roacetic  acid  (pH  2.0),  and  each  fraction  was  examined 
for  the  presence  of  immunoreactive  calcitonin  by  im- 
munoblotting.  Each  immunoreactive  fraction  was  finally 
purified  on  the  same  column  with  a  solvent  of  a  different 
pH  (ammonium  acetate  buifer,  pH  4.6). 

The  fractions  were  lyophilized  and  a  small  portion  of 
each,  or  synthetic  salmon  calcitonin  ( 1  ng- 1  ^g),  was  dis- 
solved in  10  ^1  of  a  mixture  of  0.1  M  Na2CO3  (pH  9.5) 
and  methanol  (4:1,  v/v),  and  blotted  onto  an  Immobilin 
PVDF  transfer  membrane  (Millipore  Co.  Ltd.,  Tokyo). 
The  membrane  was  soaked  in  100%i  methanol  for  3  s, 
and  washed  3  times  in  10  mM  phosphate-buffered  saline 
(pH  7.2)  containing  0.05%  Tween  20  (PBST)  for  5  min. 
The  membrane  was  washed  twice  more  in  PBST  con- 
taining 1%  normal  goat  serum,  and  then  three  times  again 
in  PBST.  The  membrane  was  then  incubated  with  an 
antiserum  raised  against  salmon  calcitonin  (Sasayama  et 
al..  1989)  (1/40,000  dilution)  for  2  h  at  room  temperature. 
The  unbound  antiserum  was  removed  by  three  washes  in 
PBST,  and  the  membrane  was  immunostained  with  a 
Vectastain  ABC  kit  (Vector  Laboratories,  California)  ac- 
cording to  the  protocol  included  with  the  kit. 

A  portion  of  purified  ray  calcitonin  was  subjected  to 
reduction  and  S-carboxymethylation.  as  reported  previ- 
ously (Takei  et  al..  1989),  and  further  purified  by  reverse 
phase  HPLC.  The  amino  acid  sequence  of  the  purified 
peptide  was  determined  with  a  protein  sequencer  (Applied 
Biosystems,  Model  470A/120A).  The  sequence  thus  de- 
termined was  verified  by  the  amino  acid  analysis  (15), 
and  by  coelution  of  the  purified  and  synthetic  peptides  in 
reverse-phase  HPLC  with  two  different  solvent  systems 
(Takei  el  al.,  1990).  The  ray  calcitonin  was  synthesized 
by  a  peptide  synthesizer  (Applied  Biosystems,  Model 
430A)  as  reported  previously  (Takei  et  til..  1989).  The 


correct  sequence  of  the  synthetic  peptide  was  confirmed 
by  amino  acid  analysis,  and  by  the  sequencer. 

Results 

At  first,  1/10  of  the  crude  acid  extract  of  ray  ultimo- 
branchial  glands  was  subjected  to  reverse  phase  HPLC. 
and  several  fractions  showed  immunoreactivity  to  the  an- 
tibody raised  against  salmon  calcitonin  (Fig.  2).  Each  pos- 
itive fraction  was  further  chromatographed  with  a  solvent 
of  different  pH,  and  only  one  immunoreactive  peak  was 
detected  from  one  of  the  positive  fractions  (Fig.  3).  No 
immunoreactive  material  was  recovered  from  the  other 
fractions.  The  height  of  the  peak  was  equivalent  to  12.2 
nmoles  of  salmon  calcitonin.  Thus,  the  ultimobranchial 
gland  of  the  ray  contains  at  least  60  nmoles/g  tissue  of 
calcitonin.  The  amino  acid  sequence  of  the  purified  ma- 
terial was  determined  by  sequencer  (Fig.  4). 

The  ray  calcitonin  was  also  purified  from  the  remaining 
9/10  of  the  crude  extract.  This  material  was  then  reduced 
and  S-carboxymethylated,  and  1/10  of  the  carboxymeth- 
ylated  peptide  was  subjected  to  amino  acid  analysis  to 
verify  the  sequence.  The  ray  calcitonin  was  composed  of 


E 
c 
o 

CM 
CNJ 


o> 
o 

c 

I 

b 

CO 


0     i 


10  15  20 

Fraction  number 


25 


30 


Figure  2.  Reverse  phase  HPLC  on  an  ODS-120T  column.  Sample, 
crude  acid  extract  of  ultimobranchial  glands  of  the  ray,  flow  rate,  I  ml/ 
min;  fraction  size,  2  ml/tube.  Solvent  system:  linear-gradient  elution 
from  20  to  80%  CH,CN  in  0.1%  trifluoroacetic  acid  for  60  min.  The 
immunoreactive  calcitonin  (ir-CT)  was  quantified  by  scores  from  0  to 
5.  Arrows  indicate  the  fraction  within  which  ray  calcitonin  was  eluted. 


CALCITONIN  FROM  CARTILAGINOUS  RAY 


487 


0.8 


0.6 


I 

o 

CO 
CM 


0.4 


0.2 


60 


50   t 

m 


40    o 


30 


20 


40 


60 


Time  (mm) 


Figure  3.  Reverse  phase  HPLC  on  an  ODS- 1 20T  column.  Sample, 
fraction  12  in  Figure  2;  flow  rate.  1  ml/min.  Solvent  system:  linear- 
gradient  elution  from  solvent  A  (H:O  :  CH,CN  :  1  M  NH4OAc,  pH 
4.6  =  72  :  8  :  1.  v/v)  to  B  (H,O  :  CH3CN  :  1  M  NH4OAc.  pH  4.6 
=  25  :  100  :  1.  v/v)  for  40  min.  Fraction  was  collected  at  each  peak. 
Immunoreactivity  appeared  only  in  the  peak  marked  by  the  arrow. 


32  amino  acid  residues:  Asp,  2.0  (2);  Glu,  4.6  (4);  CM- 
Cys,  1.3  (2);  Ser,  3.0  (3);  Gly,  2.2  (2);  His,  1.0  (1);  Arg, 
l.'l  (l);Thr,  4.2(4);  Ala,  2.2(2);  Pro,  1.1  (1),  Val.  2.4  (3). 
He.  1.0  (1),  Leu.  4.2  (4),  Lys,  2.0  (2);  the  numbers  in  pa- 
rentheses were  deduced  from  the  sequencing.  Since  the 
cysteine  residue  was  undetectable  without  carboxyme- 
th\  lation  in  a  sequencer,  the  carboxymethylated  material 
was  also  subjected  to  the  sequencer  and  the  presence  of 
cysteine  residues  at  the  first  and  the  seventh  position  was 
confirmed.  The  amidation  of  the  proline  residue  at  the 
C-terminus  was  determined  by  co-chromatography  with 
a  synthetic  peptide  in  reverse-phase  HPLC. 

Discussion 

In  this  study,  a  large  amount  of  calcitonin  (more  than 
60  nmoles/g  tissue)  has  been  detected  in  the  ultimobran- 
chial  glands  of  the  ray  (Dasyatis  akajei),  and  sequenced. 
This  is  the  first  elasmobranch  calcitonin  to  have  been 
characterized. 

In  mammals,  calcitonin  is  a  hypocalcemic  hormone 
that  inhibits  the  reabsorption  of  calcium  from  the  bone 
(Friedman  and  Raisz,  1965).  Among  non-mammals,  cal- 
citonin-like  immunoreactivity  has  been  detected  in  se- 
lected species  from  all  classes,  and  calcitonin  has  been 
isolated  in  a  bird  and  teleosts  (Niall  et  al.,  1969;  Van 
Noorden  and  Pearse,  1971;  Noda  and  Narita,  1976;  Tis- 
serand-Jochem  et  a!.,  1977;  Sasayama  el  al,,  1984;  Treil- 
hou-Lahille  et  al.,  1984;  Homma  et  al.,  1986),  but  the 


physiological  roles  of  the  hormone  in  these  species  are 
not  fully  understood.  Because  a  hypocalcemic  effect  is 
not  common  in  non-mammalian  species  (Pang  et  al., 
1980).  cartilaginous  fishes,  which  appear  to  have  large 
amounts  of  calcitonin,  may  be  good  material  with  which 
to  investigate  those  roles  of  the  hormone  that  have  been 
retained  throughout  the  vertebrates.  The  use,  in  such 
studies,  of  the  native  hormone,  now  available,  is  important 
because  mammalian  hormones  often  have  little  biological 
effect  in  fishes  (Takei  et  al.,  1989,  1990). 

The  calcitonins  sequenced  to  date  can  be  classified  into 
three  groups  according  to  their  structural  similarity  (Fig. 
1 ).  The  sequence  homology  among  the  calcitonins  within 
each  group  is  88-94%  (Table  I).  Ray  calcitonin  is  appar- 
ently more  homologous  to  non-mammalian  calcitonins, 
but  the  homology  is  less  than  that  within  any  of  the  groups. 
In  particular,  the  calcitonins  from  fowl  and  ray  are  more 
similar  than  the  two  types  of  mammalian  calcitonins. 
Non-mammalian  calcitonins  generally  have  greater  hy- 
pocalcemic effects  in  the  rat  than  do  mammalian  calci- 
tonins (Homma  et  al..  1986).  Indeed,  our  preliminary 


1000 


v   K 


15          10         15         20        25        30 

Cycle  number 

Ray  CT     CTSLSTCVVGKLSQQLHKLQNIQRTDVGAATP-NH2 

Figure  4.  Automatic  sequencer  analysis  of  the  purified  peak  of  ray 
calcitonin  immunoreactivity  shown  in  Figure  3.  The  yield  of  phenyl- 
thiohydantoin-denvitized  (PTH)  amino  acid  is  plotted  for  each  cycle  of 
Edman  degradation.  The  cystine  residues  (C)  at  cycles  1  and  7  were  not 
determined  in  this  analysis,  which  was  carried  out  without  prior  car- 
boxymethylation  (see  Results).  The  complete  amino  acid  sequence,  finally 
verified  by  amino  acid  analysis  and  by  co-chromatography  with  synthetic 
peptide,  is  set  out  below  the  plot. 


488 


V.  TAKEI  ET  AL. 


Table  I 

The  sequence  homolot;\-  <>l  amino  acids  fahove  *)  and  of  nucleotides 
(helow*/  hetnven  two  calcitonins  from  dillcrcni  species 

Ray    Fowl     Eel    Salmon     Rat     Human     Pig    Ox    Sheep 


Ra>     * 

78   75 

66 

38 

34 

31 

31 

31 

Fowl 

*    94 

88 

50 

47 

44 

44 

41 

Eel 

* 

94 

53 

50 

41 

41 

44 

Salmon 

74 

* 

59 

56 

41 

44 

41 

Rat 

66 

60 

* 

94 

47 

47 

44 

Human 

68 

60 

91 

* 

44 

44 

41 

Pig 

- 

- 

- 

- 

* 

91 

88 

Ox 

_ 

- 

- 

- 

- 

* 

94 

Sheep 

- 

- 

- 

- 

- 

- 

* 

Numbers  are  homologies  expressed  in  terms  of  percentage.  -;  not 
examined.  For  nucleotide  sequences,  see  Craig  el  nl..  1982;  Rosenteld 
el  al.  1984;  Lasmoles  et  al..  1985;  Poeschl  et  al .  1987. 


results  show  that  when  rats  are  injected  with  1  pmol  of 
ray  calcitonin,  the  plasma  calcium  concentration  de- 
creases by  15.3  ±  1.5%  after  30  min,  whereas  injection 
with  the  same  dose  of  human  and  porcine  calcitonin 
causes  a  decrease  of  9.8  ±  1.2%  and  3.5  ±  0.7%,  respec- 
tively (n  =  10  in  each  case).  Thus,  ray  calcitonin  is  ap- 
parently more  hypocalcemic  in  the  rat  than  mammalian 
calcitonins.  Ray  calcitonin  may  have  a  clinical  applica- 
tion, as  is  the  case  for  eel  calcitonin  (Orimo,  1979). 

Acknowledgments 

The  authors  are  grateful  to  Dr.  David  A.  Price  of  The 
Whitney  Laboratory,  University  of  Florida,  for  the  critical 
reading  of  this  manuscript.  We  also  thank  Miss  S.  Nishida 
for  artwork.  This  investigation  is  supported  in  part  by  the 
Terumo  Foundation. 

Literature  Cited 

Brewer,  H.  B.,  Jr.,  and  R.  Ronan.  1969.     Amino  acid  sequence  of  bovine 

calcitonin.  Biochemistry  63:  940-947. 
Craig,  R.  K.,  L.  Hall,  M.  R.  Kdbrooke,  J.  Allison,  and  I.  Maclntyre. 

1982.     Partial  nucleotide  sequence  of  human  calcitonin  precursor 

mRNA  identifies  flanking  cryptic  peptides.  Nature  295:  345-347. 
Friedman,  J.,  and  L.  G.  Raisz.  1965.     Thyrocalcitonin:  inhibition  of 

bone  resorption  in  tissue  culture.  Science  150:  1465-1467. 
Homma,  T.,  M.  Watanabe,  S.  Hirose,  A.  Kanai.  K.  Kangawa,  and  H. 

Matsuo.  1986.     Isolation  and  determination  of  amino  acid  sequence 

of  chicken  calcitonin  I  from  chicken  ultimobranchial  glands.  ./ 

Biochem.  100:  459-467. 
Lasmoles,  F.,  A.  Jullienne,  F.  Day,  S.  Minvielle,  G.  Milhaud,  and 

M.  S.  Moukhtar.  1985.     Elucidation  of  the  nucleotide  sequence  of 


chicken  calcitonin  mRNA:  direct  evidence  for  the  expression  of  a 
lower  vertebrate  calcitonin-like  gene.  EAIBO  J.  4:  2603-2607. 

Neher,  R.,  B.  Riniker.  \V.  Rittel,  and  H.  Zuber.  1968.  Menschliches 
calcitonin.  III.  Struktur  von  calcitonin  M  und  D.  Hel\:  Chun  Ada 
51:  1900-1905. 

Niall,  H.  D.,  H.  T.  Keutmann,  D.  H.  Copp,  and  J.  T.  Potts,  Jr. 
1%9.  Amino  acid  sequence  of  salmon  ultimobranchial  calcitonin. 
Proe  Nail  Acini.  Sci.  i'SA  64:  771-778. 

Noda,  T.,  and  K.  Narita.  1976.  Amino  acid  sequence  of  eel  calcitonin. 
./  Biochem  79:  353-359. 

Orimo,  H.  1979.  Clinical  application  of  calcium-regulating  hormone. 
am.  Knilocrmol.  28:  269-274. 

Pang,  P.  K.  1 .,  A.  D.  Kenny,  and  C.  Oguro.  1980.  Evolution  of  en- 
docrine control  of  calcium  regulation.  Pp.  323-356  in  Evolution  of 
I  'ertebrale  Endocrine  Systems.  P.  K.  T.  Pang,  and  A.  Epple.  eds. 
Texas  Tech.  Press,  Lubbock. 

Poeschl,  K.,  I.  I.indley,  E.  Hofer,  J.  M.  Seifert,  W.  Brunowsky,  and  J. 
Besemer.  1987.  The  structure  of  procalcitonin  of  the  salmon  as 
deduced  from  its  cDNA  sequence.  FEBS  Lett.  226:  96-100. 

Potts,  J.  T.,  Jr.,  H.  D.  Niall,  H.  T.  Keutmann,  H.  B.  Brewer.  Jr.,  and 
I..  J.  Deftos.  1968.  The  amino  acid  sequence  of  porcine  thyrocal- 
citonin.  Proc.  Nat/.  Acad.  Sci.  i'SA  59:  1321-1328. 

Potts,  J.  T.,  Jr.,  H.  D.  Niall,  H.  T.  Keutmann,  and  R.  M.  I.equin. 
1 97 1 .  Chemistry  of  calcitonin;  species  variation  plus,  structure-ac- 
tivity relations  and  pharmacologic  implications.  Pp.  1 2 1  - 1 27  in  Cal- 
cium Parathyroid  Hormone  and  the  Calcitonin.  R.  V.  Talmage  and 
P.  L.  Munson,  eds.  Excerpta  Medica,  Amsterdam. 

Raulais,  D.,  J.  Hagaman,  D.  A.  Ontjes,  R.  L.  Lundblad,  and  H.  S.  King- 
don.  1976.  The  complete  amino-acid  sequence  of  rat  thyrocalci- 
tonm.  Eur.  J.  Biochem.  64:  607-61 1. 

Rosenfeld,  M.  G.,  S.  G.  Amara,  and  R.  M.  Evans.  1984.  Alternative 
RNA  processing  events  as  a  critical  developmental  regulatory  strategy 
in  nonendocrine  gene  expression.  Biochem.  Soc.  Symp.  49:  27-44. 

Sasayama,  Y.,  C.  Oguro,  R.  Yui,  and  A.  Kanbegawa.  1984.  Im- 
munohistochemical  demonstration  of  calcitonin  in  ultimobranchial 
glands  of  some  lower  vertebrates.  Zoo/.  Sci.  1:  755-758. 

Sasayama,  Y.,  K.  Matsuda,  C.  Oguro.  and  A.  Kambegawa.  1989.  1m- 
munohistochemical  study  of  the  ultimobranchial  gland  of  chum 
salmon  fry.  7.ool.  Set.  6:  607-610. 

Takei,  Y.,  A.  Takahashi,  T.  X.  Watanabe,  K.  Nakajima,  and  S.  Sakaki- 
bara.  1989.  Amino  acid  sequence  and  relative  biological  activity 
of  eel  atrial  natnuretic  peptide.  Biochem.  Biophys.  Res.  Commtin 
164:  537-543. 

Takei,  Y.,  A.  Takahashi,  T.  X.  Watanabe,  K.  Nakajima,  S.  Sakakibara, 
T.  Takao,  and  Y.  Shimonishi.  1990.  Amino  acid  sequence  and  rel- 
ative biological  activity  of  a  natnuretic  peptide  isolated  from  eel  brain. 
Biochem  Biophys.  Res.  Commun.  170:883-891. 

Tisserand-Jochem,  E.  M.,  A.  Eyquem,  J.  Peignoux-Deville,  and  C.  Cal- 
mettes.  1977.  Calcitonine  du  corps  ultimobranchial  de'  Anguilles 
(Anxuillti  anxuilla  L.):  localisation  cytologique  par  immunofluores- 
cence  indirecte  a  1'aide  d'un  anticorps  humain  anti-calcitonine  de 
Saumon.  C.  R  Acad.  Sci.  Set:  D  258:  81-84. 

Treilhou-Lahille,  F.,  A.  Jullienne,  M.  Aziz,  A.  Beaumont,  and  M.  S. 
Moukhtar.  1984.  Ultrastructural  localization  of  immunoreactive 
calcitonin  in  the  two  cell  types  of  the  ultimobranchial  gland  of  the 
common  toad  (Bujo  bufo  L.).  Gen.  Cornp.  Endocrinol.  53:  241-251. 

Van  Noorden,  S.,  and  A.  G.  E.  Pearse.  1971.  Immunofluorescent  lo- 
calization of  calcitonin  in  the  ultimobranchial  gland  of  Rana  tem- 
porana  and  Rana  pipiens.  Histochemie  26:  95-97. 


Reference:  Biol  Bull  180:  484-445.  (June,  1991) 


Carbon  Budgets  for  Two  Species  of  Benthonic 
Symbiont-Bearing  Foraminifera 


B.   H.  TER  KUILE1  *  AND  J.  EREZ2 

{The  Interuniversity  Institute  o/'Eilat,  The  Hebrew  University  of  Jerusalem, 

P.  O.  Box  469,  Eilat  88103,  Israel,  and  ^-Department  of  Geology, 

The  Hebrew  University  of  Jerusalem,  Jerusalem  91904.  Israel 


Abstract.  Carbon  budgets  are  presented  for  two  sym- 
biont-bearing  foraminifera:  Amphistegina  lobifera,  a  per- 
forate species,  and  the  imperforate  species  Ainphisoms 
hempriehii.  Both  species  have  a  potential  for  autotrophy 
with  respect  to  carbon,  because  the  translocation  from 
symbionts  to  host  is  sufficient  to  account  for  the  increase 
in  measured  biomass.  Experimentally  determined  feeding 
rates  exceed  the  supposed  amount  of  food  retained  as 
calculated  by  balancing  the  budget  by  a  factor  of  up  to 
ten.  When  feeding  does  not  occur,  the  carbon  budget  of 
A.  lobifera  is  almost  exactly  balanced,  whereas  the  budget 
of  A.  hempriehii  can  be  balanced  within  the  precision  of 
the  measurements.  Carbon  for  calcification  by  .4.  lobifera 
is  initially  concentrated  in  an  internal  pool  that  derives 
approximately  10%  of  its  content  from  organic  matter 
respired  by  the  host.  Carbon  of  organic  origin  was  not 
incorporated  into  the  skeleton  of  A.  hempriehii. 

Introduction 

Carbon  budgets  have  been  constructed  for  various  in- 
vertebrates bearing  algal  symbionts,  such  as  corals  (Mus- 
catine  et  ai.  1981,  1984;  Falkowski  el  a/.,  1984)  and 
zoanthids  (Steen  and  Muscatine,  1984),  but  not  for  fo- 
raminifera. One  of  the  reasons  for  this  is  that  carbon  bud- 
gets can  only  be  formulated  once  the  flows  of  carbon  and 
the  mechanisms  involved  in  directing  these  flows  are 
known  qualitatively.  Then  a  scheme  integrating  all  fluxes 
can  be  drawn  up,  so  that  research  aimed  at  quantifying 
these  fluxes  can  be  properly  interpreted.  Earlier  research 


Received  19  April  1989;  accepted  8  January  1991. 
*  Present  address:  ICP-TROP  74.39,  Avenue  Hippocrate  74.  B-1200 
Brussels,  Belgium. 


within  this  framework  showed  that  the  perforate  and  im- 
perforate groups  of  foraminifera  have  widely  different 
mechanisms  for  uptake  of  inorganic  carbon  and  for  cal- 
cification (ter  Kuile  and  Erez,  1987,  1988;  ter  Kuile  et  ill., 
1989).  Therefore,  two  conceptually  different  budgets  must 
be  constructed  for  foraminifera:  one  for  a  representative 
of  the  perforate,  and  one  for  an  imperforate  species.  The 
species  we  have  chosen  for  this  study  are:  Amphistegina 
lobifera  (perforate)  and  Amphisorus  hempriehii  (imper- 
forate). 

In  earlier  studies  on  carbon  budgets  of  symbiont-bearing 
calcifying  systems,  the  contribution  of  the  symbionts  to 
the  carbon  requirements  of  the  host  was  considered  a  key 
feature  of  the  host-symbiont  relationship  and  was,  there- 
fore, often  emphasized.  Determination  of  the  carbon 
translocation  from  symbionts  to  host  is  difficult  because 
i*  involves  measurements  within  an  organism.  Another 
disputed  parameter  is  the  relative  contribution  of  feeding 
to  the  carbon  requirements  of  the  host.  Lee  and  coworkers 
(Lee  and  Bock,  1976;  Lee  et  ai.  1980)  estimated  that,  in 
foraminifera,  carbon  from  feeding  exceeds  carbon  from 
photosynthesis  by  a  factor  of  10,  but  ter  Kuile  et  al.  (1987) 
found  a  ratio  of  0.5-2.  Feeding  rates  are  difficult  to  mea- 
sure because  feeding  is  episodic,  and  because  egested  algae 
do  not  resuspend  well.  At  least  in  foraminifera,  feeding 
seems  to  provide  nutrients  rather  than  carbon  (ter  Kuile 
et  al..  1987).  Hence,  minimum  feeding  rates  can  be  esti- 
mated by  calculating  the  nutrient  requirements  of  the  host 
symbiont-system  by  assuming  a  constant  ratio  of  carbon 
to  nutrients.  One  purpose  of  this  study  is  to  estimate  the 
two  uncertain  parameters:  translocation  of  carbon  from 
symbionts  to  host,  and  the  contribution  of  feeding  to  the 
carbon  budget.  The  calculated  values,  estimated  by  bal- 
ancing the  budget  so  that  no  carbon  is  unaccounted  for. 


489 


490 


B.  H.  TER  KU1LE  AND  J.  EREZ 


are  used  as  a  control  on  the  experimentally  obtained 
values. 

Based  on  stable  isotope  experiments  with  corals  and 
foraminifera,  Goreau  (1977)  and  Erez  (1977,  1978)  sug- 
gested that  inorganic  carbon  is  initially  taken  up  in  an 
internal  inorganic  carbon  pool.  According  to  this  view, 
some  respired  carbon  of  organic  origin  may  be  taken  up 
by  the  inorganic  pool  and  afterwards  incorporated  into 
the  skeleton.  Later,  experimental  evidence  was  found  for 
the  existence  of  the  inorganic  carbon  pool  in  perforate, 
but  not  in  imperforate  foraminifera  (ter  Kuile  and  Erez, 
1987,  1988).  Such  pools  have  not  been  included  in  the 
earlier  proposed  budgets  for  corals  (Falkowski  et  al,  1984). 
We  believe,  however,  that  the  internal  inorganic  carbon 
pool  is  important  for  overall  carbon  cycling,  at  least  in 
perforate  foraminifera.  Therefore,  the  second  purpose  of 
this  study  is  to  understand  the  role  of  the  pool  in  the 
carbon  cycling  of  the  perforate  species. 

The  following  observations  have  to  be  taken  into  ac- 
count while  formulating  carbon  budgets  for  foraminifera. 
First,  the  important  taxonomical  differences  between  the 
perforate  and  imperforate  groups  of  foraminiferal  species 
are  reflected  in  widely  different  calcification  mechanisms 
(ter  Kuile  el  al,  1989).  Therefore  two  different  models 
are  proposed,  one  for  each  group. 

Perforate  species 

Inorganic  carbon  (Ci)  is  initially  taken  up  from  seawater 
in  one  flow  in  the  form  of  bicarbonate.  In  the  cytoplasm. 
CO2  is  photoassimilated  by  the  symbionts,  and  CO3=  is 
concentrated  in  the  internal  inorganic  carbon  pool  (here- 
after called  "pool"),  which  serves  for  calcification  only 
and  not  for  photosynthesis.  About  10%  of  the  carbon  in- 
corporated into  the  skeleton  consists  of  carbon  originally 
photoassimilated  by  the  symbionts  and  respired  by  the 
host.  Feeding  seems  to  provide  nutrients,  phosphate  and 
nitrogen  compounds,  rather  than  carbon,  to  the  host- 
symbiont  system.  (Leutenegger,  1977;  Leutenegger  and 
Hansen,  1979;  ter  Kuile  and  Erez,  1987,  1988;  ter  Kuile 
etai,  1987,  1988,  1989). 

Imperforate  species 

Inorganic  carbon  is  taken  up  from  seawater  in  two  sep- 
arate flows  that  do  not  interfere  with  each  other.  Carbonate 
is  taken  up  by  a  diffusion-limited  process  into  vacuoles 
where  calcification  occurs.  The  symbionts  use  either  CO: 
or  HCO3~.  Some  carbon  derived  from  feeding  may  be 
assimilated  in  the  host  organic  matter,  but  not  in  the  skel- 
eton. Internal  recycling  of  respired  carbon  from  organic 
origin  into  the  skeleton  does  not  occur  (Hemleben  et  al., 
1986;  ter  Kuile  and  Erez,  1987,  1988;  ter  Kuile  et  a/., 
1987,  1989). 


We  present  carbon  budgets  for  two  species  of  forami- 
nifera, based  on  rates  determined  in  a  large  number  of 
experiments;  some  of  the  data  were  obtained  from  other 
studies  (ter  Kuile  and  Erez,  1987.  1988;  ter  Kuile  et  al.. 
1987). 

Materials  and  Methods 

Amphistegina  lobifera  (perforate)  and  Amphisorus 
hemprichii  (imperforate)  were  collected  from  Halophila 
sp.  plants,  24  h  before  each  experiment.  We  checked  the 
foraminifera  for  viability  by  observing  their  overnight  up- 
ward mobility  in  glass  jars  (ter  Kuile  and  Erez,  1984, 
1987).  The  budget  presented  for  A.  lobifera  comprises 
measurements  of  specimens  with  average  weights  of  66 
to  72  ng  (Table  I);  specimens  of  A.  hemprichii  weighed 
385  jug  on  average,  ranging  from  242  to  523  ^g-  Long- 
term  kinetic  and  pulse-chase  experiments  involving  I4C 
tracer  techniques  (ter  Kuile  and  Erez,  1987,  1988)  were 
used  throughout  the  study.  Incubations  were  carried  out 
in  100  ml  erlenmeyer  flasks  near  a  north-oriented  window 
in  natural  light/dark  cycles.  The  maximum  light  intensity 
was  750  nE  m~2s "',  which  corresponds  to  a  depth  of  15 
m,  similar  to  the  depth  of  the  sample  location.  Twenty 
to  40  mg  of  organisms  were  used  for  each  incubation. 

Determination  of  compartment  biomass 

The  biomass  of  the  following  compartments  was  mea- 
sured: total  dry  weight,  biomass  of  the  organic  matter, 
dry  weight  of  the  skeleton,  and  the  carbon  content  of  in- 
ternal inorganic  carbon  pool.  Determinations  of  protein 
and  chlorophyll  content  were  used  to  estimate  symbiont 
biomass. 

Total  dry  weight  of  foraminifera  was  determined  with 
a  Cahn  25  electrobalance.  Biomass  of  organic  matter  was 
measured  as  the  additional  weight  of  a  Nuclepore  (0.4  n) 
filter  on  which  the  paniculate  organic  matter  of  a  sample 
whose  shell  was  dissolved  in  8.5%  H,PO4  had  been  col- 
lected. The  organic  matter  of  Amphistegina  lobifera  was, 
on  the  average,  8.0%  (±0.6,  n  =  48)  of  the  total  dry  weight, 
and  Amphisorus  hemprichii  contained  5.2%  (±0.6)  organic 
matter.  The  dry  weight  of  the  skeleton  was  determined 
by  substracting  the  dry  weight  of  the  organic  matter  from 
the  total  dry  weight.  The  internal  inorganic  carbon  pool 
size  of  A.  lobifera  was  measured  by  I4C  radiotracer  meth- 
ods, in  combination  with  pulse-chase  experiments  (ter 
Kuile  and  Erez,  1988). 

The  protein  content  of  finely  crushed,  dried  specimens 
was  determined  by  the  Lowry  method,  as  modified  by 
Peterson  ( 1 977).  The  contribution  of  the  symbionts  to  the 
organic  matter  could  be  estimated  from  the  chlorophyll 
content  measured  after  extraction  in  methanol  (Strickland 


CARBON  BUDGETS  IN  FORAMINIFERA 


491 


and  Parsons,  1972);  this  was  possible  because  we  found 
no  change  in  the  chlorophyll  to  protein  ratio  in  samples 
obtained  at  depths  less  than  35  m.  Sizes  of  compartments 
are  given  in  ^g  C/mg  foram  (total  dry  weight  of  skeleton 
and  organic  matter). 

Fluxes  between  the  compartments 

The  following  five  fluxes  were  measured;  the  methods 
used  were  exactly  those  of  the  papers  cited  in  each  case. 
( 1 )  The  uptake  of  inorganic  carbon  from  the  medium- 
consisting  of  photoassimilation,  uptake  into  the  skeleton, 
uptake  into  the  pool  and,  by  addition,  total  uptake — was 
measured  as  H'4CO3  uptake  (ter  Kuile  and  Erez,  1987, 
1988).  (2)  Translocation  of  photosynthates  from  sym- 
bionts  to  host  was  calculated  from  pulse-chase  experi- 
ments (ter  Kuile  and  Erez,  1987).  (3)  Incorporation  of 
metabolic  carbon  (initially  taken  up  photosynthetically) 
into  the  skeleton  was  also  derived  from  pulse-chase  ex- 
periments, (ter  Kuile  and  Erez,  1987,  1988).  (4)  Respi- 
ration was  again  derived  from  pulse-chase  experiments. 
(5)  Uptake  and  rejection  of  carbon  derived  from  feeding 
on  algae  in  the  environment  (not  their  own  symbionts) 
was  determined  in  time-course  and  pulse-chase  experi- 
ments as  previously  reported  (ter  Kuile  el  «/.,  1987).  All 
rates  are  given  in  ^g  C/mg  foram/24  h  in  a  natural  light/ 
dark  cycle. 


AMPHISTEGINA   LOBIFERA 


Table  1 

Rales  of  carbon  tixalion  ami  pnnl  sue  in  Amphistegina  lobifera 
in  LI  natural  light/dark  cycle 


Figure  1.  Carbon  budget  for  Amphistegina  lobifera.  The  compart- 
ments and  the  fluxes  between  them  were  qualitatively  described  in  earlier 
studies  (see  Introduction).  The  names  and  sizes  of  different  compartments 
are  given  in  large  letters  and  numbers;  the  names  of  processes  and  the 
amounts  of  carbon  transferred  are  in  small  lettering.  Open  arrows  indicate 
transfer  of  inorganic  carbon,  striped  arrows  indicate  transfer  of  organic 
carbon,  and  closed  arrows  indicate  the  active  transport  of  carbonate. 
Numbers  framed  in  compartment  corners  indicate  daily  increase  in  size 
of  that  compartment.  Units:  sizes  of  compartments  in  jig  C/mg  foram. 
Rates  of  fixation  and  transfer,  and  daily  increase,  in  /ig  C/mg  foram/24 
h  in  a  natural  light/dark  cycle. 


Total 

Photo 

Skeleton 

Pool  size 

Org.  wt. 

(j/g  C/mg 

(Mg) 

(fig,  C/mg  foram/24 

h) 

loram) 

66 

4.9 

2.0 

2.9 

68 

4.8 

1.6 

3.2 

2.5' 

68 

5.1 

1.7 

3.4 

70 

5.5 

1.9 

3.6 

72 

3.9 

1.2 

2.7 

Average: 

4.8 

1.7 

3.2 

*  In  experiments  not  reported  here,  a  pool  size  of  2.2  to  2.9  ng  C/mg 
foram  was  measured  in  foraminifera  with  an  organism  weight  of  about 

70  Mg. 

Abbreviations:  Org.  wt  =  organism  weight:  Total  =  total  carbon  uptake; 
Photo  =  carbon  uptake  for  photosynthesis  by  the  symbionts:  Skeleton 
=  carbon  uptake  for  calcification;  Pool  size  =  carbon  content  of  the 
internal  inorganic  carbon  pool  for  calcification.  The  standard  deviation 
of  a  large  number  of  measurements  on  identical  samples,  which  were 
made  using  our  methodology,  was  around  5%  of  the  reported  value  (ter 
Kuileand  Erez,  1987). 


Results 

Budget  descriptions 

The  carbon  budget  for  Amphistegina  lobifera  is  pre- 
sented in  Figure  I .  The  compartments  are  defined  and 
their  size  determined  as  described  above  in  Materials  and 
Methods.  The  existence  of  the  fluxes  between  them  was 
demonstrated  in  earlier  studies  (ter  Kuile  and  Erez,  1987, 
1988;  ter  Kuile  et  ai.  1989.  see  Introduction).  The  uptake 
rates  used  to  construct  the  budget  are  given  in  Table  I. 
The  organic  compartment  makes  up  8.0%  of  the  total  dry 
weight.  About  half  of  organic  dry  weight  is  carbon  (Sver- 
drup  el  a!..  1942;  Parsons  and  Takahashi,  1973),  which 
amounts  to  40  j*g  C/mg  foram.  The  ratio  of  chlorophyll 
to  protein  is  roughly  1:39  (Table  II);  a  usual  ratio  for  algae 
is  1:10  (Parsons  and  Takahashi,  1973).  Thus,  the  sym- 
bionts comprise  about  one  quarter  of  the  total  organic 
matter.  The  organic  matter  compartments  of  symbionts 
and  host  contain  about  10  and  30  ^g  C/mg  foram,  re- 
spectively. The  skeleton  comprises  92%  of  the  total  dry 
weight,  which  amounts  to  1 10  MB  C/mg  foram.  The  in- 
organic carbon  pool  size  (ter  Kuile  and  Erez,  1988)  de- 
pends on  the  calcification  rate,  which  in  turn  depends  on 
the  size  of  the  specimens.  For  specimens  of  roughly  70 
jig,  a  pool  size  of  approximately  2.5  jtg  C/mg  foram  (Table 
I)  was  found  in  the  experiments  performed  for  this  study. 
In  other  studies  we  found  similar  values  (2.2-2.9  ^g  C/ 
mg  foram)  (ter  Kuile  and  Erez,  1988). 

Total  uptake  of  inorganic  carbon  (Ci)  by  Amphistegina 
lobifera  was,  on  average,  4.8  Mg  C/mg  foram/24  h  (Table 


492 


B.  H.  TER  KU1LE  AND  J.  EREZ 


Table  II 

Protein  and  chlorophyll  measurements  of  Amphistegma  lobifera 
and  Amphisorus  hemprichii  (duplicate  measurements 

mi  ditlerent  si:e  groups) 


Protein 

Organism 

(Mg/mg 

weight 

foram  ) 

(Mg) 

A  lobifera 

33.92 

340 

32.10 

340 

40.05 

60 

39.40 

60 

A  .  hemprichii 

20.88 

1107 

17.10 

1107 

26.30 

283 

23.49 

283 

Ratio  protein/ 
chlorophyll 


Organism 
weight 


.4.  lobifera 


A.  hemprichii 


39.7 
37.4 
40.1 
45.2 
34.7 
40.8 
41.4 


>250 

75-250 

<75 

>2000 

>2000 

<500 

<500 


I).  Under  the  experimental  conditions,  specimens  of  the 
size  range  used  in  this  study  (around  70  Mg)  grew  at  a 
daily  rate  of  about  3%/day.  This  rate  was  determined  op- 
tically, by  converting  size  increase  to  weight  increase  (ter 
Kuile  and  Erez,  1984),  and  by  the  incorporation  of 
UCO32~  into  the  skeleton.  Approximately  1.7  (1.2-2.0) 
Mg  C/mg  foram/24  h  net  is  fixed  photosynthetically  by 
the  symbionts.  The  chlorophyll:protein  ratio  does  not 
change  with  size  (Table  II),  indicating  that  the  symbionts 
grow  in  proportion  to  the  organic  matter.  When  growing 
at  a  rate  of  3%  a  day,  the  symbionts  need  0.3  Mg  C/mg 
foram/24  h  for  growth.  Hence,  a  net  amount  of  1 .4  Mg  C/ 
mg  foram/24  h  will  be  available  for  translocation  to  the 
host.  Calculations  based  on  the  results  of  pulse-chase  ex- 
periments indicate  a  transfer  of  1 .3  Mg  C/mg  foram/24  h. 
At  the  measured  growth  rate,  the  host  needs  0.9  Mg  C/mg 
foram/24  h  for  growth.  Transfer  of  respired  Ci  to  the  skel- 
eton amounts  to  0.3  Mg  C/mg  foram/24  h.  Loss  of  respired 
Ci  to  the  environment  is  roughly  0.2  Mg  C/mg  foram/24 
h.  Incorporation  into  the  skeleton  is  3.2  Mg  C/mg  foram/ 
24  h  (Table  I).  This  carbon  is  initially  concentrated  in  the 
pool  which,  in  turn,  derives  0.3  Mg  C/mg  foram/24  h  from 
respired  carbon  (see  above)  and,  by  balance.  2.9  Mg  C/mg 
foram/24  h  is  taken  up  directly  from  seawater.  When  no 
feeding  occurs,  the  budget  is  balanced  with  respect  to  up- 
take, growth,  and  respiration.  During  feeding  experiments, 
large  amounts  of  labeled  algae  (up  to  14  Mg  C/mg  foram/ 


24  h)  were  rapidly  ingested,  but  most  of  this  food  was 
egested  in  organic  form  within  24  h  (ter  Kuile  ct  til..  1987). 
Approximately  8%  of  the  carbon  in  the  food  was  respired. 
Less  than  2%  of  the  label  taken  up  through  feeding  was 
incorporated  into  the  skeleton  (ter  Kuile  el  al,  1987). 
Feeding  rates  depend  on  the  conditions  during  preincu- 
bation  and  the  availability  of  suitable  food.  Therefore,  the 
values  given  in  Figure  1  must  be  considered  minimum 
and  maximum  rates,  rather  than  long-term  averages. 
Consequently,  the  value  for  respiration  is  at  a  minimum 
when  no  feeding  occurs  and  organisms  grow  slowly,  and 
at  a  maximum  when  feeding  rates,  and  thus  growth  rates, 
are  high. 

Amphisorus  hemprichii  budget 

A  similar  budget  for  the  carbon  cycling  of  Amphisorus 
hemprichii  is  presented  in  Figure  2.  This  budget  differs 
strongly,  not  only  quantitatively,  but  qualitatively  as  well, 
from  the  budget  of  A.  lobifera.  reflecting  the  widely  dif- 
ferent calcification  mechanisms  found  in  perforate  and 
imperforate  foraminifera.  respectively  (see  Introduction). 
Because  of  the  large  size  range,  the  variation  in  the  data 
was  also  large  (Table  III).  The  organic  matter  was  5.2% 
of  the  total  weight  (dry  weight/dry  weight).  Symbiont  bio- 
mass  is  about  one  quarter  of  the  total  organic  matter, 
estimated  from  the  chlorophyll:protein  ratio  (1:40.5  ±  4.3; 
ter  Kuile  and  Erez,  1984;  this  study.  Table  II).  When  con- 
verted to  carbon  weight,  the  sizes  of  the  organic  com- 
partments are  7.5  Mg  C/mg  foram  for  symbionts,  and  22.5 
Mg  C/mg  foram  for  the  host.  Skeleton  contains  1 1 3  Mg  C/ 
mg  foram.  A.  hemprichii  does  not  contain  an  internal 
inorganic  carbon  pool  for  calcification  (ter  Kuile  and  Erez, 
1987,  1988). 


AMPHISORUS    HEMPRICHII 


Figure  2.  Carbon  budget  for  Amphisorus  hemprichii.  This  budget 
differs  from  that  of  Amphistegina  lobifera  due  to  differences  in  the  cal- 
cification mechanisms  (see  Introduction).  Units  as  in  Figure  1. 


CARBON  BUDGETS  IN  FORAM1NIFERA 


493 


Table  III 

Rales  of  carbon  fixation  in  Amphisorus  hemprichii  in  a  natural  lix 
dark  cycle.  No  internal  inorganic  carbon  pool  is  observed 
in  A.  hemprichii 


Org.  wt. 

Total 

Photo 

Skeleton 

fag) 

(Mg  C/mg  foram/24  h) 

243 

2.5 

1.5 

1.0 

362 

2.9 

1.1 

1.8 

409 

2.5 

1.0 

1.5 

523 

3.7 

1.7 

2.0 

Average 


1.6 


116 

3.3 

1.2 

2.1 

149 

3.0 

1.2 

1.8 

3000 

2.2 

1.3 

0.9 

3500 

2.8 

1.6 

1.2 

*  Light  and  heavy  specimens,  not  used  in  Figure  2. 
Abbreviations  and  units  as  in  Table  I. 


In  the  experiments  for  this  budget,  a  net  average  of  1.3 
Hg  C/mg  foram/24  h  (Table  III)  was  fixed  photosynthet- 
ically.  Because  Amphisorus  hemprichii  grew  roughly  1 .5%/ 
day  in  the  laboratory  (ter  Kuile  and  Erez,  1984,  1987), 
the  symbionts  and  the  host  organic  matter  compartments 
increase  0.1  and  0.3  /ug  C/mg  foram/24  h,  respectively. 
By  balance,  0.9  ng  C/mg  foram/24  h  should  be  respired. 
The  respiration  rate  calculated  from  pulse-chase  experi- 
ments (ter  Kuile  and  Erez,  1987)  was  1.1  Mg  C/mg  foram/ 
24  h.  Translocation,  estimated  from  pulse-chase  experi- 
ments, was  1.5  ng  C/mg  foram/24  h.  The  calculated  rate 
is  1.2  ng  C/mg  foram/24  h,  which  is  within  the  precision 
of  the  measurement.  Up  to  15  ng  C/mg  foram/24  h  is 
taken  up  through  feeding  (ter  Kuile  et  at..  1987).  In  one 
pulse-chase  experiment,  25%  of  the  amount  initially  in- 
gested was  still  present  after  one  week.  Thus,  feeding  may 
contribute  considerable  amounts  of  reduced  carbon  for 
the  growth  of  A.  hemprichii.  About  half  of  the  food  that 
was  not  retained  was  respired,  and  the  rest  was  egested, 
both  in  roughly  equal  rates  of  about  1.5  ^g  C/mg  foram/ 
24  h.  Egestion  is  difficult  to  measure  in  A.  hemprichii, 
because  the  fecal  pellets  do  not  resuspend.  At  present,  the 
budget  is  not  balanced  with  respect  to  carbon  derived  from 
feeding,  because  the  estimated  egestion  is  too  low  (ter 
Kuile  el  ai,  1987).  To  balance  the  budget,  the  egestion 
rate  should  be  12  ng  C/mg  C/24  h.  Uptake  into  the  skel- 
eton was,  on  the  average,  1.5  (1.0-2.0)  ^g  C/mg  foram/ 
24  h  (Table  III).  Even  though  uptake  for  photosynthesis 
and  calcification  occurs  in  roughly  equal  rates,  about  four 
times  more  carbon  is  accumulated  in  the  skeleton  than 
in  the  organic  matter,  because  most  of  the  photosynthates 
are  respired.  Specimens  weighing  less  than  150  ng  have 
higher  rates  of  calcification  than  of  photosynthesis. 


whereas  specimens  heavier  than  3000  ^g  have  lower  rates 
of  calcification  than  of  photosynthesis.  In  the  medium 
range,  the  calcification:photosynthesis  ratio  was  constant, 
roughly  1:1  (comparison  in  Table  III). 

Discussion 

Carbon  budgets  for  benthonic  symbiont-bearing  fora- 
minifera  can  best  be  compared  to  a  similar  budget  for 
corals  developed  by  Falkowski  and  coworkers  ( 1984).  The 
relative  sizes  of  the  compartments  in  corals  and  forami- 
nifera  differ:  in  Amphislegina  lobifera,  the  symbionts,  host 
organic  matter,  and  skeleton  contain  approximately  7, 
20,  and  73%  of  the  total  carbon,  respectively.  For  Am- 
phisorus hemprichii  these  numbers  are:  5,  16,  and  79%. 
Corals  contain  about  1-2%  organic  matter  (dry  weight/ 
dry  weight)  (Erez,  1978),  which  amounts  to  5%'  of  the 
carbon  in  organic  form  and  95%  in  the  skeleton.  The 
symbionts  constitute  only  3.7-4.5%  of  the  total  organic 
matter  (Falkowski  et  a/..  1984),  giving  a  final  distribution 
of  0.2%,  4.8%,  and  95%  for  carbon  in  the  symbionts,  host 
organic  matter,  and  the  skeleton. 

In  calcareous  algae,  70-90%  of  the  total  dry  weight  is 
CaCO3  (Pentecost,  1980);  therefore,  the  ratio  of  carbon 
in  the  organic  matter  to  carbon  in  the  skeleton  is  about 
1:1.  Coccolithophores  form  coccoliths  depending  on  the 
environmental  conditions,  and  may  shed  them  after  for- 
mation. The  relative  amount  of  carbon  in  the  skeleton  is 
therefore  difficult  to  estimate,  but  it  is  probably  1 : 1  as  well 
(Sikes  el  ai.  1980:  Van  der  Wai,  1984). 

Therefore,  corals  contain  the  least  amount  of  organic 
carbon  per  unit  of  inorganic  (calcareous)  carbon,  fora- 
minifera  are  intermediate,  and  calcareous  algae  contain 
the  most.  This  suggests  that  corals  need  to  take  up  fewer 
nutrients  in  the  form  of  nitrogen  or  phosphorous  com- 
pounds from  their  surroundings  per  unit  total  carbon 
(both  organic  and  calcareous),  foraminifera  need  more, 
and  calcareous  algae  require  still  more  than  the  symbiotic 
systems.  This  has  consequences  for  the  carbon  cycling  of 
foraminifera,  because  feeding  may  be  the  primary  source 
of  nutrients,  at  least  in  A.  lobifera  (ter  Kuile  et  a/.,  1987). 
Determination  of  feeding  rates  was  the  least  reliable  mea- 
surement of  our  budget,  because  feeding  is  a  discontinuous 
process,  and  because  egestion  cannot  be  measured  well. 
Assuming  that  foraminifera  obtain  all  their  nutrients  from 
food  and  have  the  same  C:N:P  ratio  as  the  food,  the 
amount  of  nutrients  retained  can  be  estimated.  The  total 
daily  increase  of  organic  matter  of  A.  lobifera,  host  and 
symbionts,  is  1 .2  ng  C/mg  foram/24  h,  which  can  be  pro- 
vided by  photoassimilation  by  the  symbionts.  The  max- 
imum feeding  rate  is  about  ten  times  higher.  Thus,  about 
10%  of  the  nutrients  present  in  the  food  are  retained, 
while  almost  all  of  the  carbon  derived  from  feeding  is 
respired  or  egested  (ter  Kuile  et  ai.  1987).  The  general 


494 


B.  H.  TER  KUILE  AND  J.  EREZ 


"black  box"  observation  that  the  efficiency  of  retention 
between  trophic  layers  is  usually  around  10%  further 
supports  the  validity  of  the  high  experimental  rates. 
Based  on  the  carbon  budget,  we  expect  that  long-term 
feeding  rates  are  much  lower,  unless  feeding  is  very  in- 
efficient, or  the  feeding  efficiency  varies  with  the  food 
concentration. 

The  photosynthetic  rates  of  Amphistegina  lobifera  and 
Amphisorus  hemprichii  measured  in  this  study  are  similar 
to  those  found  by  Erez  (1978),  but  are  an  order  of  mag- 
nitude lower  than  those  of  planktonic  foraminifera  (Erez, 
1983;  Jorgensen  ct  at.,  1985).  Photosynthetic  rates  of  cor- 
als, when  normalized  to  the  total  weight  of  the  organism, 
are  similar  (Erez,  1978),  or  much  lower  (Falkowski  et  al, 
1984).  Total  weight  may  not  be  a  useful  normalization 
factor  for  corals.  The  symbionts  of  foraminifera  and  light- 
adapted  corals  translocate  sufficient  reduced  carbon  to 
the  host  to  sustain  respiration  and  growth  (Jacques  and 
Pilson,  1980;  Muscatine  et  al.,  1981,  1984;  Falkowski  et 
al.,  1984;  Davies,  1984;  Edmonds  and  Spencer  Davies, 
1986).  Therefore,  these  systems  have  a  potential  for  au- 
totrophy  with  respect  to  carbon,  but  not  to  nutrients  that 
must  be  provided  by  feeding  (Falkowski  et  al..  1984;  ter 
Kuile  et  til..  1987).  Besides  nutrients,  planktonic  fora- 
minifera and  shade-adapted  corals  require  additional  re- 
duced carbon  from  feeding  (Falkowski  et  al.,  1984;  Jor- 
gensen et  al..  1985).  Excretion  of  mucus  by  corals  has 
been  well  documented  (Grassland  et  al.,  1980a,  b;  Mus- 
catine et  al..  1984;  Crossland.  1987),  but  we  found  no 
evidence  that  foraminifera  lose  photosynthetically  fixed 
carbon  in  the  form  of  mucus.  Cycling  of  respired  carbon 
into  the  skeleton  has  been  demonstrated  for  both  corals 
(Crossland  et  al.,  1980a)  and  foraminifera  (ter  Kuile  and 
Erez,  1987). 

Corals  and  perforate  foraminifera  may  have  another 
common  feature,  the  internal  inorganic  carbon  pool.  First 
predicted  in  corals  to  explain  the  stable  isotope  compo- 
sition of  the  calcium  carbonate  skeleton  by  Goreau  (1977), 
this  pool  was  demonstrated  experimentally  in  perforate 
foraminifera,  but  not  in  imperforate  species  (ter  Kuile 
and  Erez,  1988).  This  pool  functions  solely  as  a  carbon 
reservoir  for  calcification  in  which  carbonate  is  concen- 
trated in  an  energy-dependent  process  (ter  Kuile  et  al., 
1989).  More  uptake  in  the  pool  occurs  when  metabolic 
rates  supported  by  symbiont  activity  are  high,  but  no  car- 
bon from  the  pool  is  photoassimilated  (Fig  1 ).  This  cor- 
relation between  uptake  by  the  pool  and  photosynthetic 
activity  may  explain  the  lighter  than  expected  isotopic 
composition  of  rapidly  photosynthesizing  corals  and  fo- 
raminifera (Erez,  1978).  The  occurrence  of  the  same  phe- 
nomenon in  both  classes  of  organisms  suggests  that  the 
pool  of  corals  may  operate  similarly  to  that  of  perforate 
foraminifera. 


Acknowledgments 

This  study  was  supported  by  the  United  States-Israel 
Binational  Science  Foundation,  Project  3418/83.  The  au- 
thors wish  to  thank  Drs.  Z.  Reiss  and  B.  Luz  for  fruitful 
discussions. 

Literature  Cited 

Crossland,  C.  J.  1987.  In  situ  release  of  mucus  and  DOC-lipid  from 
the  corals  Acropora  variabilis  and  Stylophora  pistillata  in  different 
light  regimes.  Coral  Reefs  6:  35-42. 

Crossland,  C.  J.,  D.  J.  Barnes,  T.  Cox,  and  M.  Devereux. 
I980a.  Compartmentation  and  turnover  of  organic  carbon  in  the 
staghorn  coral  Acropora  formosa.  Mar.  Biol  59:  181-187. 

Crossland,  C.  J.,  D.  J.  Barnes,  and  M.  A.  Borowitzka.  1980b.  Diurnal 
lipid  and  mucus  production  in  the  staghorn  coral  Acropora  aatminata. 
Mar.  Biol.  60:  81-90. 

Davies,  P.  S.  1984.  The  role  of  zooxanthellae  in  the  nutritional  energy 
requirements  of  Poci/lopera  evdouxi.  Coral  Reels  2:  181-1 86. 

Edmunds,  P.  J.,  and  P.  Spencer  Davies.  1986.  An  energy  budget  for 
Porites  porites  (Scleractinia)  Mar.  Biol.  92:  339-347. 

Erez,  J.  1977.  Influence  of  symbiotic  algae  on  the  stable  isotope  com- 
position of  hermatypic  corals:  a  radioactive  tracer  approach.  Proc. 
3rd.  Int.  Coral  ReefSymp.  2:  563-569. 

Erez,  J.  1978.  Vital  effect  on  stable-isotope  composition  seen  in  fo- 
raminifera and  coral  skeletons.  Nature  273:  199-202. 

Erez,  J.  1983.  Calcification  rates,  photosynthesis  and  light  in  planktonic 
foraminifera.  Pp.  307-313  in  Biomineralizalion  and  Biological  Metal 
Accumulation,  P.  Westbroek  and  E.  J.  de  Jong,  eds.  Rcidel.  Dordrecht. 

Falkowski,   P.  G.,  Z.   Dubinsky,   L.  Muscatine,  and  J.  W.   Porter. 

1984.  Light  and  the  bioenergetics  of  a  symbiotic  coral.  Bioscience 
34:  705-709. 

Goreau,  T.  J.  1977.  Coral  skeletal  chemistry:  physiological  and  envi- 
ronmental regulation  of  stable  isotopes  and  trace  metals  in  Monlaslrea 
annitltins.  Proc.  R  Soc.  Loml.  B  196:  291-315. 

Uemleben,  Ch.,  O.  R.  Anderson,  W.  Berthold,  and  M.  Spindler. 
1986.  Calcification  and  chamber  formation  in  Foraminifera — a  brief 
overview.  Pp.  237-249  in  Special  Volume  30:  Biomineralizalion  in 
Lower  Plants  and  Animals.  B.  S.  C.  Leadbeater  and  R.  Riding,  eds. 
The  Systematics  Association.  London. 

Jacques,  T.  G.,  and  M.  E.  Q.  Pilson.  1980.  Experimental  ecology  of 
the  temperate  scleractinian  coral  Astrangia  danae.  I.  Partition  of 
respiration,  photosynthesis  and  calcification  between  host  and  sym- 
bionts. Mar.  Biol.  60:  167-178. 

Jorgensen,    B.    B.,    J.    Erez,    N.    P.    Revsbech,    and    Y.    Cohen. 

1985.  Symbiotic  photosynthesis  in  a  planktonic  foraminiferan. 
Globigerinoides  sacculiter  (Brady),  studied  with  microelectrodes. 
Limnol.  Oceanogr.  30:  1253-1267. 

Kuile,  B.  ter,  and  J.  Erez.  1984.     In  situ  growth  rate  experiments  on 

the  symbiont-bearing  foraminifera  Amphistegina  lobijera  and  Am- 
phisorus licmprkitii.  J  Faram.  Res.  14:  262-276. 
Kuile,  B.  ter.  and  J.  Erez.  1987.     Uptake  of  inorganic  carbon  and  internal 

carbon  cycling  in  benthonic  symbiont-beanng  foraminifera.  Mar 

Biol.  94:499-510. 
Kuile,  B.  ter,  J.  Erez,  and  J.  J.  Lee.  1987.     The  role  of  feeding  in  the 

metabolism  of  larger  symbiont-bearing  foraminifera.  Symbiosis  4: 

335-350. 
Kuile,  B.  ter,  and  J.  Erez.  1988.     The  size  and  function  of  the  internal 

inorganic  carbon  pool  of  the  foraminifer  Amphistegina  lobijera.  Mar. 

Biol  99:  481-487. 
Kuile,  B.  ter,  A.  Kaplan,  and  J.  J.  Lee,  1988.     Uptake  of  inorganic 

carbon  by  Fragilariu  shiloi.  symbiont  of  the  foraminifer  Amphistegina 

lobifera.  Symbiosis  6:  225-236. 
Kuile,  B.  ter,  J.  Erez,  and  E.  Padan.  1989.     Mechanisms  for  the  uptake 


CARBON  BUDGETS  IN  FORAMINIFERA 


495 


of  inorganic  carbon  by  two  species  of  symbiont-bearing  foraminifera. 

Mar.  Biol  103:  241-251. 
Lee,  J.  J.,  and  \V.  D.  Bock.  1976.     The  importance  of  feeding  in  two 

species  of  soritid  foraminifera  with  algal  symbionts.  Bull.  Mar.  Sci. 

26:  530-537. 
Lee,  J.  J.,  M.  E.  McEnery,  and  J.  R.  Garrison.  1980.     Experimental 

studies  of  larger  foraminifera  and  their  symbionts  from  the  Gulf  of 

Elat  on  the  Red  Sea.  J.  Foram.  Res  10:  31-47. 
Leutenegger,  S.  1977.     infrastructure  de  Foraminiferes  perfores  et  im- 

perfores  ainsi  que  leurs  symbiotes.  Cah.  Micropaleontol.  (CNRS, 

Paris)*:  1-52. 
Leulenegger,  S.,  and  H.  J.  Hanson.  1979.     Infrastructure  and  radiotracer 

studies  of  pore  function  in  foraminifera.  Mar.  Biol.  54:  11-16. 
Muscatine,  I...  I..  R.  McCloskey,  and  R.  E.  Marian.  1981.     Estimating 

the  daily  contribution  of  carbon  from  zooxanthellae  to  coral  animal 

respiration.  Limnol.  Oceanogr.  26:  60 1  -6 1  I . 
Muscatine,  L.,  P.  G.  Kalkowski,  J.  W.  Porter,  and  /..  Dubinsky, 

198-4.     Fate  of  photosynthetic  fixed  carbon  in  light-  and  shade- 
adapted  colonies  of  the  symbiotic  coral  Stylophora  pislillala.  Proc. 

R  Soc.  Land.  B  222:  181-202. 


Parsons,  T.  R.,  and  M.  I  akahashi.  1973.     Biological  Oceunographical 

Processes  Pergamon  Press.  New  York.  1 86  pp. 
Pentecost,  A.  1980.     Calcification  in  plants.  Int.  Rev.  Cyt.  62:  1-25. 
Peterson,  G.  I,.  1977.     A  simplification  of  the  protein  assay  method  of 

Lowry  et  al.  which  is  more  generally  applicable.  Anal.  Bioclieiu  83: 

346-356. 
Sikes,  S.,  R.  D.  Roer,  and  K.  M.  Wilbur.  1980.     Photosynthesis  and 

coccolith  formation:  Inorganic  carbon  sources  and  net  inorganic  re- 
action of  deposition.  Limnol.  Oceangr.  25:  248-261. 
Steen,  R.  G.,  and  L.  Muscatine.  1984.     Daily  budgets  of  photosyn- 

thetically  fixed  carbon  in  symbiotic  zoanthids.   Biol.   Bull    167: 

477-487. 
Strickland,  J.  D.,  and  R.  Parsons.  1972.     A  Practical  Handbook  of 

Sean-ater  Analysis.  2nd  ed.  Fisheries  Research  Board  of  Canada. 

310pp. 
Sverdrup,  II.  II.,  M.  W.  Johnson,  and  R.  11.  Fleming.  1942.     The  Oceans, 

Their  Physics,  Chemistry  and  General  Biology.  Prentence-Hall,  En- 

glewood  Cliffs,  NJ.  1087  pp. 
\Val,  P.  van  der  1984.     Calcification  in  two  species  of  coccolithophorid 

algae.  Gua  Papers  Geol.  20:  1 1 3  pp. 


Reference:  Bin!.  Hull  180:496-504.  (June,  1991) 


The  Induction  of  Carbonic  Anhydrase  in  the  Symbiotic 
Sea  Anemone  Aiptasia  pulchella 

VIRGINIA  M.  WEIS* 
Department  of  Biology,  University  of  California,  Los  Angeles,  California  90024 


Abstract.  The  activity  and  nature  of  carbonic  anhydrase 
(CA,  EC  4.2.1.1.)  was  measured  and  described  in  the 
tropical  sea  anemone  Aiptasia  pulchella.  The  hypothesis 
that  high  CA  activity  in  animal  tissue  is  induced  by  the 
presence  of  symbiotic  algae  was  tested.  CA  activity  was 
positively  correlated  with  the  number  of  symbiotic  di- 
noflagellates  (zooxanthellae)  present.  CA  activity  in  apo- 
symbiotic  anemone  tissue  was  2.5  times  lower  than  that 
in  control  symbiotic  animals  or  in  aposymbiotic  animals 
repopulated  with  algae.  Polyclonal  antisera  against  human 
CA  were  used  to  probe  for  the  presence  of  CA  in  both 
symbiotic  and  aposymbiotic  anemone  tissue,  and  in 
freshly  isolated  and  cultured  zooxanthellae.  The  resulting 
immunoblots  showed  one  band  with  a  molecular  weight 
of  30  kDa  in  symbiotic  animal  tissue  and  control  mam- 
malian CA  lanes,  no  bands  in  the  aposymbiotic  animal 
lanes,  and  one  band  at  a  molecular  weight  of  22.5  kDa 
in  freshly  isolated  and  cultured  zooxanthellae  lanes.  Be- 
cause no  22.5  kDa  band  was  detected  in  the  symbiotic 
animal  tissue  lanes,  the  high  CA  activity  found  in  sym- 
biotic animal  tissue  is  considered  to  be  due  to  the  induc- 
tion of  animal  enzyme  by  the  presence  of  algae.  The  lack 
of  any  band  in  the  aposymbiotic  lanes  further  supports 
the  hypothesis  that  CA  activity  in  A.  pulchella  is  induced 
by  the  presence  of  algae. 

Introduction 

Symbiotic  dinoflagellates  ("zooxanthellae")  residing  in 
vacuoles  within  cells  of  marine  cnidarians  exhibit  a  high 
rate  of  photosynthesis  (Falkowski  et  ai,  1984).  When  this 
rate  exceeds  the  respiration  rate  of  the  association,  the 
algae  must  draw  on  inorganic  carbon  (C,)  from  the  sea- 


Received  2  October  1990:  accepted  25  February  1991. 
*  Present  address:  Department  of  Biological  Sciences.  University  of 
Southern  California,  Los  Angeles,  CA  90089. 


water  pool  to  satisfy  the  high  carbon  demand.  CO:  is  the 
C,  species  preferred  as  a  substrate  for  carbon  assimilation 
by  ribulose  bisphosphate  carboxylase/oxygenase  (RU- 
BISCO)  in  the  zooxanthellae.  Yet  at  an  ambient  pH  of 
8.2-8.3,  C,  in  seawater  is  present  mostly  as  HCOj  .  Ad- 
ditionally, the  movement  of  HCOr  across  unstirred 
boundary  layers  and  the  several  animal  and  algal  mem- 
branes to  the  site  of  photosynthesis  could  be  relatively 
slow  (Kerby  and  Raven,  1985). 

Weis  et  ai  (1989)  hypothesize  that  the  supply  of  CO2 
for  photosynthesis  in  algal/cnidarian  symbioses  is  aug- 
mented by  the  presence  in  the  cnidarian  tissue  of  carbonic 
anhydrase  (CA,  EC  4.2.1.1.),  an  enzyme  that  catalyzes 
the  inter-conversion  of  HCOr  and  CO2.  In  the  22  species 
of  cnidarians  examined,  CA  activity  in  the  animal  tissue 
of  symbiotic  species  was,  on  average,  29  times  higher  than 
in  non-symbiotic  species.  In  the  symbiotic  species,  CA 
activity  in  the  animal  fraction  was  2-3  times  higher  than 
that  in  the  algae.  These  results  suggest  that  CA  activity  in 
animal  tissue  is  related  to  the  presence  of  zooxanthellae. 

Two  other  findings  indicate  that  CA  activity  in  sym- 
biotic animal  tissue  is  related  to  the  presence  of  algae. 
First,  CA  activity  is  correlated  with  habitat  irradiance  in 
colonies  of  the  coral  Stylophora  pistillata  (Weis  et  ai, 
1989).  5.  pistillata  from  high  light  habitats  exhibited  sig- 
nificantly higher  rates  of  CA  activity  than  did  those  living 
at  lower  light  levels.  Second,  there  are  spatial  differences 
in  CA  activity  within  the  same  individual  (Weis  et  ai, 
1989).  Column  tissue  of  the  anemone  Condylactis  gigan- 
tea,  which  lacks  symbionts,  had  very  low  activity  com- 
pared to  the  tentacle  tissue  which  contains  symbionts. 

In  this  study  I  present  further  evidence,  from  work  on 
symbiotic  and  aposymbiotic  Aiptasia  pulchella.  of  a  pos- 
itive correlation  between  the  CA  activity  in  animal  tissue 
and  the  number  of  zooxanthellae  present.  Additionally, 
I  use  the  immunoblot  technique  to  show  that  high  CA 


496 


CARBONIC  ANHYDRASE  IN  A  SEA  ANEMONE 


497 


activity  in  symbiotic  animal  tissue  is  the  result  of  induction 
in  the  animal  tissue  by  the  presence  of  the  algae. 

Materials  and  Methods 

Maintenance  of  experimental  organisms 

A  clone  of  the  anemone  Aiplasia  pulcliella  (Java  clone) 
was  maintained  in  laboratory  in  aquaria  or  large  finger 
bowls  containing  Millipore-filtered  seawater  (MFSW)  ob- 
tained from  Santa  Monica  Bay.  For  at  least  14  days  prior 
to  experimentation,  anemones  were  kept  in  a  Precision 
incubator  at  25°C  at  an  irradiance  of  40  /iE-rrr2-s~' 
on  a  12  h  light/dark  cycle  unless  otherwise  specified. 
Throughout  the  experiments  the  anemones  were  fed  Ar- 
temia  nauplii  once  weekly,  and  the  finger  bowls  were 
cleaned  and  the  water  was  changed  daily. 

Zooxanthellae  isolated  from  the  Java  clone  were  grown 
in  ASP-8A  medium  (Guillard  and  Keller,  1984)  in  25  1 
clear  plastic  carboys.  The  carboys  were  incubated  at  room 
temperature  at  an  irradiance  of  approximately  60 
juE  •  m  :  •  s~'  ( 16  h  light/8  h  dark  cycle).  The  cultures  were 
aerated  with  air  passed  through  a  bacterial  air  filter  (Gel- 
man  Bacteria  air  vent).  One  carboy  would  yield  approx- 
imately 10  ml  of  wet  packed  cells  after  approximately  75 
days.  The  cells  were  collected  by  centrifugation  and  stored 
at-70°C. 

Aposymbiotic  and  repopulaled  animals 

A  three  part  study  was  designed  to  measure  CA  activity 
in  symbiotic,  aposymbiotic,  and  newly  repopulated  sym- 
biotic animals.  Fifteen  animals  were  incubated  under 
controlled  maintenance  conditions  for  14  days.  Five  an- 
imals were  then  assayed  for  CA  activity,  as  described  be- 
low, which  provided  values  for  control  symbiotic  animal 
tissue. 

Ten  anemones  were  subjected  to  a  low  temperature 
shock,  a  treatment  that  rendered  them  aposymbiotic 
(Steen  and  Muscatine,  1987).  The  anemones  were  placed 
in  the  dark  at  4°C,  in  pre-cooled  MFSW,  for  4  h  and 
subsequently  incubated  at  25 °C  in  the  dark.  As  a  result 
of  this  treatment,  A.  pulchella  expelled  99%  of  its  algae 
within  a  week.  To  insure  that  virtually  all  of  the  algae 
were  expelled,  these  ten  anemones  were  then  maintained 
in  the  dark  at  25°C  for  ten  weeks. 

After  ten  weeks  in  the  dark,  five  aposymbiotic  anemo- 
nes were  placed  in  the  light  (12  h  light/dark  at  40 
^E  •  m  :  •  s  ' )  for  repopulation  by  zooxanthellae.  For  the 
first  two  weeks  of  the  repopulation  period,  one  symbiotic 
anemone  was  placed  in  the  bowl  with  the  aposymbiotic 
anemones  as  a  potential  algae  donor.  After  seven  weeks, 
the  repopulated  anemones  had  regained  their  former 
brown  color,  indicating  the  presence  of  algae  and  were 
subsequently  assayed  for  CA  activity. 


Change  in  CA  activity  with  a  change 
in  numbers  of  algae 

The  kinetics  of  loss  of  algae  and  concomitant  change 
in  CA  activity  in  the  anemone  fraction  of  the  association 
was  also  quantified.  Forty-two  anemones  were  placed,  for 
two  weeks,  under  the  control  conditions  described  above. 
Three  anemones  were  assayed  for  CA  activity  and  sampled 
for  algal  numbers  on  day  one.  and  another  three  were 
kept  in  control  conditions  for  the  duration  of  the  exper- 
iment (32  days)  and  then  sampled  at  the  end.  These  sets 
were  the  controls.  The  remaining  anemones  were  divided 
into  two  groups.  Half  of  the  anemones  were  subjected  to 
a  cold  shock  in  the  same  fashion  as  described  above  and 
subsequently  maintained  in  the  dark  at  25°C.  The  other 
half  was  simply  placed  in  the  dark  at  25°C  (dark  treated). 
Three  anemones  in  each  group,  cold  shock  and  dark 
treated,  were  sampled  for  algal  number  and  assayed  for 
CA  activity  after  3,  6,  10,  17,  24,  and  32  days  in  the  dark. 

Separation  of  algae  and  anemone  tissue 
for  the  CA  assay 

Anemones  were  homogenized  in  a  hand-held  Teflon- 
glass  tissue  homogenizer  in  3.5  ml  of  MFSW  chilled  to 
2°C.  The  homogenate  was  transferred  to  a  10  ml  conical 
centrifuge  tube  and  centrifuged  at  900  X  g  for  1  min  to 
separate  animal  tissue  (supernatant)  from  algae  (pellet). 
There  was  no  evidence  that  the  supernatant  was  contam- 
inated with  algae.  The  animal  tissue  supernatant  was  de- 
canted and  diluted  1:1  (v/v)  with  cold  25  mM  veronal 
buffer  (2°C),  containing  5  mM  EDTA,  5  mM  dithiothre- 
itol  (DTT)  and  10  mAl  MgSO4,  adjusted  to  pH  8.2  (mod- 
ified from  Graham  and  Smillie,  1976).  At  this  point,  the 
animal  tissue  supernatant  was  ready  for  the  CA  assay. 

Algal  pellets  were  resuspended  in  MFSW  and  centri- 
fuged several  times,  which  removed  most  of  the  residual 
anemone  debris.  The  algae  were  then  resuspended  in  1 
ml  of  10%  formalin  in  MFSW,  refrigerated  and  saved. 
Cell  numbers  were  determined  with  a  haemacytometer 
and  indexed  to  the  weight  of  soluble  anemone  protein 
(determined  as  described  below). 

In  vitro  assay  for  CA  activity 

The  in  vitro  CA  assay  is  described  in  detail  by  Weis  et 
al.  (1989).  The  CA  activity  in  animal  homogenates  was 
measured  by  the  decrease  in  pH,  resulting  from  the  hy- 
dration  of  CO:  to  HCO3  and  FT,  after  the  addition  of 
substrate.  CO:-saturated  distilled  H2O  served  as  substrate 
and  was  prepared  prior  to  an  experiment  by  passing  gas- 
eous CO2  through  an  air-stone  in  200  ml  of  distilled  H2O 
at  2 °C  for  10  min.  The  water  was  considered  to  have  been 
saturated  when  the  pH  was  below  3.5,  and  it  was  then 
stored  in  a  tightly  stoppered  glass  flask  at  2°C. 


498 


V.  M.  WEIS 


The  assay  was  run  as  follows.  One  milliliter  of  the  butt- 
ered animal  homogenate  was  further  diluted  with  1  ml  of 
50  mM  veronal  buffer,  (adjusted  to  pH  8.2  with  1  N 
NaOH)  and  transferred  to  a  small  glass  test  tube.  The 
mixture  was  stirred  with  a  magnetically  driven  stir  bar. 
One  ml  of  substrate  was  then  added  rapidly,  and  the  de- 
crease in  pH  of  the  constantly  stirred  mixture  was  recorded 
with  a  Beckman  combination  Ag/AgCl  pH  probe  im- 
mersed in  the  mixture  and  connected  to  a  Beckman  Model 
45  pH  meter.  The  meter  was  fitted  to  an  Acorn  BBC  com- 
puter with  an  analog  to  digital  (A/D)  converter  that  con- 
verted the  meter  output  to  a  digital  record.  The  data  were 
collected  and  analyzed  by  a  customized  software  program 
(John  Lighton,  copyright  1985). 

As  a  control  for  non-specific  change  in  pH,  the  same 
procedure  was  carried  out  with  animal  homogenate  which 
had  been  heated  to  boiling  for  5  min,  and  then  cooled  to 
2°C.  This  treatment  eliminated  most  or  all  CA  activity. 
There  was  no  evidence  of  renaturation  upon  cooling.  CA 
activity  of  native  animal  homogenate  and  heat-denatured 
control  was  measured  in  triplicate.  Units  of  enzyme  ac- 
tivity were  normalized  to  the  weight  of  soluble  protein 
(Hartree,  1972)  with  bovine  serum  albumin  (Sigma) 
as  a  standard.  CA  activity  was  expressed  as  ApH 
units-  min  '  •  mg  soluble  protein"1  as  determined  from: 

(ApH  of  native  animal  homogenate 

-  ApH  of  denatured  control)- min"' 
mg  soluble  animal  protein 

Sample  preparation  for  electrophoresis 

Symbiotic  and  aposymbiotic  anemones  were  homog- 
enized in  a  2.5  ml  Teflon-glass  tissue  homogenizer,  in  an 
extraction  buffer  consisting  of  10  mA/  phosphate  buffer 
at  pH  6.8  with  1  mA/  ethylenediaminetetraacetate 
(EDTA),  5  mA/MgSO4,  5  mA/dithiothreitol  (DTT),  and 
2  mA/  phenylmethyl-sulfonyl  fluoride  (PMSF),  a  protease 
inhibitor.  The  homogenate  was  centrifuged  at  12,000  rpm 
in  an  Eppendorf  microfuge  for  7  min  to  pellet  the  zoo- 
xanthellae  and  animal  debris.  No  evidence  was  found  of 
contamination  of  the  supernatant  by  algae.  The  algal  pellet 
was  cleaned  three  times;  in  each  instance,  the  cells  were 
suspended  and  centrifuged  in  MFSW.  The  pellet  was  then 
stored  at  -70°C  until  needed.  The  slightly  milky  super- 
natant, containing  the  animal  tissue,  was  decanted  and 
stored  in  a  test  tube  on  ice.  Usually  8  animals,  each  with 
an  oral  disc  diameter  of  0.6-0.9  mm,  were  homogenized 
in  0.75  ml  of  buffer  to  yield  a  concentration  of  approxi- 
mately 4000  ng  protein/ml.  Soluble  protein  was  quantified 
using  the  method  of  Hartree  ( 1972). 

At  least  0.2  ml  of  packed  algae,  cultured  or  freshly  iso- 
lated, were  required  to  yield  enough  protein  for  gel  elec- 
trophoresis and  immunoblotting.  For  the  freshly  isolated 
algae,  many  frozen  pellets  from  different  isolations  had 


to  be  combined  to  yield  0.2  ml.  The  0.2  ml  of  thawed 
algae  were  suspended  in  5  ml  of  2%  Triton  X-100  in 
MFSW  for  10  min  to  permeabilize  and  weaken  the  cell 
wall  and  cell  membrane.  The  algae  were  alternately  cen- 
trifuged at  2000  rpm  in  a  table  top  centrifuge,  and  washed 
with  MFSW,  until  foam  from  the  Triton  was  gone  from 
the  supernatant.  The  cells  were  then  resuspended  in  0.5 
ml  of  the  extraction  buffer  with  approximately  0.3  ml  of 
425-600  j/m  diameter  glass  beads  (Sigma).  The  mixture 
was  "vortexed"  vigorously  in  a  test  tube  for  1  min  and 
centrifuged,  first  at  2000  rpm  for  1  min  in  a  table  top 
centrifuge,  and  then  at  1 2,000  rpm  for  7  min  in  a  micro- 
fuge, to  remove  the  beads,  unbroken  cells,  and  cell  wall 
debris.  The  resulting  clear,  very  deep  orange  supernatant 
was  decanted  and  stored  in  a  test  tube  on  ice.  This  tech- 
nique disrupted  approximately  70%  of  the  cells,  as  mea- 
sured by  haemacytometer  cell  counts  of  samples  before 
and  after  the  treatment,  and  produced  3500-4000  j*g  pro- 
tein/ml. 

Mammalian  CA  (Worthington  Biochemical),  used  as 
a  control,  was  dissolved  in  extraction  buffer  to  a  concen- 
tration of  500  Mg/ml.  Prestained  rainbow  molecular  weight 
markers  (Amersham)  were  used  as  standards. 

Electrophoresis  and  immunoblotting 

Immunoblots,  with  anti-CA  as  a  probe,  were  performed 
on  animal  tissue  and  zooxanthellae  to  determine  the  na- 
ture of  CA  in  the  different  fractions.  SDS-polyacrylamide 
gel  electrophoresis  (PAGE)  vyas  carried  out  using  tech- 
niques modified  from  Laemmli  (1970).  A  12.5%  resolving 
gel  and  a  4.5%  stacking  gel  were  most  commonly  used. 
Gels,  6.5  cm  long  and  0.75  mm  thick,  were  run  on  a 
Hoefer  SE  250  slab  gel  apparatus  with  continuous  cooling. 
Before  being  loaded,  the  samples  were  diluted  1 : 1  with  a 
treatment  buffer  (Laemmli,  1970)  and  boiled  for  90  s. 
Twenty  n\  of  sample  were  loaded,  equalling  approximately 
40-50  ng  of  protein/sample.  The  gels  were  run  at  a  con- 
stant voltage  (200  V)  and  were  stained  with  either  Coo- 
massie  blue  (Hames  and  Rickwood,  1987)  or  silver  nitrate 
(Johnstone  and  Thorpe,  1987). 

Electrophoretic  transfer  of  proteins  from  unstained  gels 
onto  nitrocellulose  paper  was  carried  out  in  a  Hoefer  TE 
22  transfer  apparatus  for  2  h  at  4°C  at  a  constant  current 
(200  mA)  in  a  25  mA/  Tris,  192  mA/  glycine,  and  20% 
methanol  buffer,  pH  8.3.  (Towbin  el  al,  1979).  Subse- 
quently, the  nitrocellulose  was  incubated  for  1-2  h  in  a 
blocking  buffer  of  3%  Carnation  instant  dry  milk  in  Tris 
buffered  saline  (50  mA/Tris,  150  mA/NaCl)  pH  7.4,  and 
then,  overnight,  in  the  appropriate  primary  antiserum  in 
blocking  buffer  at  room  temperature.  For  each  blot,  one 
of  two  polyclonal  antisera  was  used:  a  sheep  anti-human 
CA  [from  Bioproducts  for  Science  (BPS)]  at  a  dilution  of 
1:200,  or  a  sheep  anti-human  CA  (from  ICN)  at  1:1000. 


CARBONIC  ANHVDRASE  IN  A  SEA  ANEMONE 


499 


The  blots  were  washed  in  blocking  buffer  and  incubated 
for  2  h  in  a  1:1000  dilution  of  the  secondary  antibody, 
an  alkaline  phosphatase-conjugated,  donkey  anti-sheep 
IgG  (Sigma).  In  the  development,  nitro  blue  tetrazolium 
(NBT)  and  5-bromo-4-chloro-3-indolyl  phosphate  (BCIP) 
were  used  as  the  substrates  (Engvall,  1 980;  Johnstone  and 
Thorpe,  1987). 

Results 

CA  activity  in  symbiotic,  aposymbiotic. 
and  repopiilated  anemones 

To  determine  whether  CA  activity  in  animal  homog- 
enate  is  correlated  with  the  presence  of  algae  in  animal 
tissue,  CA  activity  was  measured  in  animal  tissue  from 
(1)  control  symbiotic  anemones,  (2)  aposymbiotic  ane- 
mones, and  (3)  repopulated  anemones.  Both  control 
symbiotic  and  repopulated  symbiotic  anemones  were  light 
brown  and  had  similar  average  CA  activities  of  1.82 
±  0.27  and  1.83  ±0.40  ApH  units  •  min  '  •  mg  protein  ', 
respectively.  In  contrast,  the  aposymbiotic  animals  were 
white,  almost  transparent,  and  had  a  significantly  lower 
average  value  of  0.75  ±0.12  ApH  units  •  min  '  •  mg  pro- 
tein -'  (Fig.  1). 

Change  in  CA  activity  with  a  change 
in  numbers  of  algae 

To  determine  whether  CA  activity  would  change  with 
a  change  in  numbers  of  algae,  anemones  were  sampled 
kinetically,  as  described  above.  The  number  of  algae  lost 
with  increasing  time  in  the  dark  was  quantified  in  both 
cold  shock  and  dark  treated  anemones  (Fig.  2).  After  just 


Control 


Aposymbiotic       Repopulated 


Figure  1 .  CA  activity  in  control  anemones,  anemones  rendered  apo- 
symbiotic by  cold  shock  treatment  and  kept  in  the  dark  for  10  weeks, 
and  anemones  rendered  aposymbiotic.  kept  in  the  dark,  and  subsequently 
reinfected  with  zooxanthellae.  Each  value  is  a  mean  ±  SD  of  the  mean 
(n  =  5).  *  =  different  from  aposymbiotic  by  P  <  .0001.  **  =  different 
from  aposymbiotic  by  .0001  <  P  <  .005  as  calculated  from  a  one  way 
ANOVA. 


in, 


rl 

E 


in- 


IOJ 


i. 


* 


I 


3  6  10  17  24  32 

Days 

Figure  2.  Number  of  zooxanthellae  *  mg  animal  protein  '  versus 
days  in  the  dark  for  control  •  dark-treated  tfl  and  cold  shocked  D  Aiptasia 
pulclwlla.  Each  value  is  a  mean  ±  SD  (n  =  3). 


3  days  of  darkness,  the  cold  shocked  anemones  contained 
less  than  one  third  as  many  algae  as  the  dark  treated 
anemones  (Table  I).  From  days  3  to  10,  cold  shock  treated 
anemones  lost  over  90%  of  their  algae,  compared  to  only 
50%  in  dark  treated  anemones.  By  the  end  of  the  32  day 
experiment,  cold  shocked  animals  had  only  about  4%  of 
the  number  of  cells/mg  animal  protein  contained  in  dark 
treated  animals  (Table  I).  The  numbers  of  algae  in  control 
anemones,  at  the  beginning  and  at  the  end  of  the  exper- 
iment, remained  high  (Table  I).  Initially,  only  cold 
shocked  anemones  had  significantly  fewer  algae  than  the 
controls,  but  by  the  end  of  the  experiment  both  dark 
treated  and  cold  shocked  anemones  had  lost  significant 
numbers  of  cells  compared  to  the  light  controls  (Table  I). 

CA  activity  in  the  animal  fraction  of  both  dark  and 
cold  shock  treated  anemones  was  measured  with  increas- 
ing time  in  the  dark  (Fig.  3).  CA  activity  in  control  animals 
at  the  beginning  and  end  of  the  experiment  were  similar. 
CA  activity  in  dark-treated  anemones  decreased  modestly, 
while  CA  activity  in  cold  shocked  anemones  decreased 
more  dramatically  (Table  II).  From  days  3  to  10,  CA  ac- 
tivity decreased  by  46%  in  cold  shocked  anemones,  but 
by  only  28%  in  dark-treated  anemones.  CA  activity  in 
cold  shocked  versus  dark-treated  animals  was  significantly 
different  only  at  day  10  (ANOVA:  .005  <  P  <  .01).  Yet 
at  day  32,  only  CA  activity  in  cold  shocked  anemones 
was  significantly  different  (ANOVA)  from  the  control 
(Table  II). 

The  CA  activity  in  the  animal  tissue  was  directly  cor- 
related with  the  number  of  algae  present  for  both  dark 


500 


V.  M.  WEIS 


Table 

Tests  lur  </;7/i7i7Jc'cs  in  ci/^tic1  numbers  between  control  anil  treated  animals 


Treatment 

#  of  algae  (X106) 
animal  protein 

Control,  3  days 
10.20  ±  1.16 

Control,  32  days 
7.50  ±  0.72 

Dark,  3  days 
Cold  shocked,  dark,  3  days 
Dark.  32  days 
Cold  shocked,  dark.  32  days 

8.72  ±  2.40 
2.53  ±  0.60 
.50  ±  0.36 
.02  ±0.01 

P>  .25 
.0001  <  /><  .005 

.000  1  <  P  <  .005 
.0001  <P<  .005 

Significance  values  from  one  way  ANOVA  tests  between  the  listed  groups,  each  with  n  =  3,  are  given  below  along  with  a  mean  ±  standard  deviation 
for  each  treatment.  The  treatment  type  is  listed  with  the  number  of  days  in  the  dark  after  the  beginning  of  the  experiment. 


and  cold  shock  treated  anemones  (Fig.  4).  Most  of  the 
lower  values  were  from  the  cold  shock  treated  anemones. 

Electrophoresis  and  immunoblotting 

To  determine  the  nature  of  CA  in  the  association,  sym- 
biotic and  aposymbiotic  anemone  tissue,  as  well  as  freshly 
isolated  and  cultured  zooxanthellae,  were  probed  for  the 
presence  of  CA  with  polyclonal  antisera  against  human 
CA.  In  the  immunoblots,  both  the  mammalian  CA  and 
symbiotic  animal  tissue  lanes  contained  one  band  with 
an  apparent  molecular  weight  of  30  kiloDaltons  (kDa) 
(Fig.  5).  One  band  with  an  apparent  molecular  weight  of 
22.5  kDa  appeared  in  the  cultured  zooxanthellae  lane, 
and  no  reaction  occurred  in  the  aposymbiotic  animal  tis- 
sue lane  (Fig.  5).  Freshly  isolated  algae  lanes  also  contained 
a  single  band  at  22.5  kDa  (data  not  shown),  suggesting 
that  their  CA  was  similar  to  that  in  the  cultured  algae. 


2.5 


~°-       1.5 
S -F 

a  is 

U|       L0 

a 


0.5- 


0.0 


10 


17 


24 


Days 

Figure  3.  CA  activity  in  animal  tissue  versus  number  of  days  in  the 
dark  for  control  •  dark-treated  S  and  cold  shocked  D  Aiptasia  pulchella. 
Each  value  is  a  mean  ±  SD  of  the  mean  (n  =  3). 


The  symbiotic  animal  and  cultured  zooxanthellae  had  dif- 
ferent relative  signal  strengths  with  the  two  antibodies 
used  (Table  III).  The  symbiotic  animal  lane  gave  roughly 
equal  signals  at  30  kDa  with  both  the  BPS  anti-CA  and 
the  ICN  anti-CA,  whereas  the  algae  at  22.5  kDa  reacted 
only  with  the  ICN  anti-CA.  Both  anti-CA  probes  labeled 
mammalian  CA  well. 

Discussion 

Evidence  for  the  correlation  oj'CA  activity 
with  the  presence  of  zooxanthellae 

The  significant  decrease  in  CA  activity  in  aposymbiotic 
versus  control  anemones  and  the  subsequent  increase  in 
repopulated  anemones  to  control  levels  (Fig.  1 )  show  that 
CA  activity  in  anemone  tissue  is  correlated  with  the  pres- 
ence of  algae.  These  findings  are  consistent  with  discovery 
of  a  spatial  relationship  between  zooxanthellae  and  CA 
activity  in  the  anemone  Condylactis  gigantea  (Weis  et  a!., 
1989).  Additionally,  the  hypothesis  that  CA  is  functioning 
in  the  delivery  of  carbon  to  the  zooxanthellae  (Weis  et 
til.,  1989)  is  further  supported  by  these  data.  Thus,  if  algae 
are  not  present,  the  supply  of  CO2  to  the  anemones  re- 
quires no  augmentation.  Although  CA  activity  is  low  in 
the  aposymbiotic  animals,  it  is  not  absent.  CA  is  present 
in  virtually  all  organisms  and  functions  in  intracellular 
pH  maintenance  (Wyeth  and  Prince,  1977). 

The  study  of  kinetics  also  reveals  a  correlation  between 
CA  activity  and  algal  numbers.  CA  activity  starts  to  de- 
crease almost  as  soon  as  the  cold  shocked  anemones  begin 
to  expel  their  algae,  and  it  stops  decreasing  when  algal 
numbers  begin  to  stabilize.  The  similarity  of  the  CA  ac- 
tivity in  cold  shocked  anemones  after  32  days  (Fig.  3)  and 
ten  weeks  (Fig.  1 )  suggests  that  the  decrease  in  CA  activity 
is  discontinued  after  32  days.  The  relatively  modest  de- 
crease in  CA  activity  over  time  in  dark  treated  anemones 
is  consistent  with  the  relative  paucity  of  algae  expelled 
from  these  anemones  compared  with  the  cold  shocked 
animal  (Fig.  2). 


CARBONIC  ANHYDRASE  IN  A  SEA  ANEMONE 
Table  II 

Tests  for  differences  in  CA  activity  helm-en  control  and  trailed  animals  


501 


Treatment 


ApH  -min  ' 
protein 


mg 


Control,  3  days 
1.332  ±0.457 


Control,  32  days 
1.248  ±0.186 


Dark,  3  days 

Cold  shock,  dark,  3  days 

Dark.  32  days 

Cold  shock,  dark,  32  days 


1.672  ±  0.694 

1.208  ±0.288 

.842  ±  0.394 

.452  ±  0.237 


P  >  .25 
P  >  .25 


.10<P<.25 
.01  <  P<  .025 


Significance  values  from  one  way  ANOVA  tests  between  the  listed  groups,  each  with  n  =  3,  are  given  below  along  with  a  mean  ±  standard  deviation 
for  each  treatment.  The  treatment  type  is  listed  with  the  number  of  days  in  the  dark  after  the  beginning  of  the  experiment. 


The  nature  oj'CA  in  A.  pulchella 

Animal  CA,  a  zinc  metalloenzyme,  has  a  molecular 
weight  of  approximately  30  kDa,  and  has  as  many  as  six 
isozymes  (Coleman,  1980;  Lindskog  et  ai.  1971;  Tashian, 
1989).  Plant  CA  has  been  less  extensively  studied,  but 
occurs  in  a  wide  variety  of  terrestrial  and  aquatic  plants 
and  algae  (Lamb,  1977;  Poincelot,  1979;  Reed  and  Gra- 
ham, 1981;  Graham  et  ai,  1984).  Plant  CA  varies  in  mo- 
lecular weight  from  about  40  to  250  kDa;  it  consists  of 
up  to  6  subunits  ranging  in  size  from  approximately  25- 
34  kDa.  Different  numbers  of  subunits  and  molecular 
weights  have  been  reported  even  for  a  single  species  (Gra- 
ham et  ai,  1984).  This  study  indicates  that  anemone  CA 
is  a  30  kDa  protein,  whereas  CA  from  freshly  isolated  or 
cultured  algae  is  either  a  22.5  kDa  protein  or  a  protein 
with  several  22.5  kDa  subunits  (Fig.  5),  a  weight  slightly 
below  the  25-34  kDa  range  reported  for  other  algae  and 
higher  plants  (Graham  et  ai.  1984).  The  successful  la- 
beling of  both  cnidarian  and  zooxanthellae  CA  with  anti- 
human  CA  indicates  that  at  least  some  portions  of  the 
enzyme  are  highly  conserved. 

Because  protein  from  freshly  isolated  algae  was  difficult 
to  obtain  (large  quantities  of  anemones  and  extensive 
cleaning  were  needed  to  yield  enough  uncontaminated 
algal  protein),  most  experiments  were  performed  on  cul- 
tured algae.  The  similar  labeling  of  cultured  and  freshly 
isolated  algae  at  22.5  kDa  suggests  that  they  have  CAs  of 
identical  molecular  weight. 

Induction  of  animal  CA  by  the  presence  of  algae 

Induction  or  deinduction  of  an  enzyme  occurs  when 
the  factors  controlling  its  synthetic  pathway  are  removed 
or  changed.  Additionally,  changes  in  rates  of  enzyme  deg- 
radation can  affect  the  relative  activity  of  an  enzyme. 
These  processes  can  take  from  minutes  to  days  to  be  man- 
ifested as  a  change  in  enzyme  activity.  Induction  of  CA 
activity  in  the  animal  tissue  in  the  presence  of  zooxan- 
thellae could  account  for  the  vastly  different  rates  of  CA 
activity  in  different  regions  of  an  individual  of  Condylactis 


gigantea  or  in  symbiotic  A.  pulchella  relative  to  aposym- 
biotic  ones.  In  this  study,  CA  activity  decreased  at  a  faster 
rate  in  the  first  ten  days  than  in  the  last  22  in  both  cold 
shock  and  dark  treated  anemones  (Fig.  3).  This  stabili- 
zation of  CA  activity  by  the  end  of  the  experiment  suggests 
that  the  putative  deinduction  or  increased  degradation  of 
the  enzyme  took  place  in  the  first  ten  days.  A  similar 
plateau  in  algal  population  size,  although  not  as  well  pro- 
nounced (Fig.  2),  remains  consistent  with  the  correlation 
of  CA  activity  and  the  presence  of  zooxanthellae. 

The  immunoblots  of  the  symbiotic  animal  tissue,  apo- 
symbiotic  animal  tissue  and  cultured  algae  (Fig.  5)  suggest 
that  high  CA  activity  in  symbiotic  animal  tissue  exhibited 
in  Figure  1  is  due  to  induction  of  CA  in  the  animal  by 
the  presence  of  the  algae,  rather  than  to  the  presence  of 
algal  CA.  In  Figure  5,  anti-CA  labeled  a  single  band  at  30 
kDa  in  symbiotic  animal  tissue,  whereas  no  such  band 
appeared  in  aposymbiotic  animals.  This  result  is  consistent 


it 

1! 

-I 
'E  a 


Log  (number  of  algac/mg  animal  prot) 

Figure  4.  CA  activity  in  animal  tissue  of  dark-treated  •  and  cold 
shocked  O  anemones  versus  log  (number  of  algae  *  mg  animal  protein  ). 
Each  point  is  a  datum  from  a  single  animal.  The  r  =  0.518. 


502 


V.  M.  WEIS 


kDa 

200.0- 
92.5\ 
69.0- 
46  -I 


30.0-  «». 

21. 5\ 

14.3 *-   •» 


II 


III 


30.0- 


22.5- 


M    A    S     Z 


MSA 


M     Z 


Figure  5.  An  SDS-polyacrylamide  gel  stained  with  Coomassie  blue  (I)  and  corresponding  immunoblots 
(II  and  III).  Blot  II  was  probed  with  BPS  anti-human  CA  and  blot  III  with  ICN  anti-human  CA.  M  =  control 
purified  mammalian  CA,  S  =  symbiotic  animal  extract.  A  =  aposymbiotic  animal  extract,  and  Z  =  cultured 
zooxanthellae  extract. 


with  the  loss  of  enzyme  activity  in  aposymbiotic  anemones 
as  compared  to  symbiotic  and  repopulated  A.  pitlche/la 
(Fig.  I ;  although  there  is  low  CA  activity  in  aposymbiotic 
anemones,  there  is  not  enough  protein  in  the  gel  to  react 
with  the  anti-CA  probe).  The  molecular  weight  of  CA 
from  freshly  isolated  and  cultured  algae  was  22.5  kDa. 
No  band  at  22.5  kDa  was  detected  in  any  symbiotic  animal 
lanes.  If  algal  CA  were  responsible  for  CA  activity  in  sym- 
biotic animal  tissue,  either  by  export  of  its  CA  to  the  peri- 
algal  space  or  further  into  the  animal  tissue,  then  it  should 
appear  on  the  gel  in  the  symbiotic  animal  tissue  lane. 

Induction  of  CA  activity  has  been  studied  in  detail  in 
mammalian  tissues  (see  Deutsch,  1987,  for  review),  but 
much  less  in  invertebrates,  plants,  and  algae.  Such  studies 
include  the  induction  of  CA  activity  during  osmotic  stress, 
to  aid  in  osmoregulation,  in  various  crustaceans  (Henry 
and  Cameron,  1983;  Wheatly  and  Henry,  1987;  Henry, 
1988).  CA  activity  can  be  induced  in  some  microalgae. 
The  chlorophytes  Chlamydomonas  reinhardtii  (e.g., 
Badger  et  ai.  1978;  Coleman  et  a/..  1984,  1985)  and 
Chlorella  vulgaris  (Hogetsu  and  Miyachi,  1979;  Tsuzuki 
et  at..  1980),  and  the  rhodophyte  Porphyridium  spp. 
(Dixonelal.,  1987;  Yagawarta/.,  1987)  show  an  increase 
in  CA  activity  when  switched  from  a  high  to  a  low  CO: 
environment. 

The  mechanism  of  induction  of  CA  activity  is  largely 
undescribed.  In  humans,  the  mechanism  varies  greatly 
depending  on  both  the  function  of  CA  and  the  tissue  or 
organ  type  (see  Deutsch,  1987,  for  some  examples).  As 
mentioned  above,  CA  is  induced  in  C.  reinhardtii  by  low 
[CO2]  but  also  by  light  (Dionisio  et  at..  1989a,  b).  In  algal/ 
cnidarian  symbioses,  any  number  of  factors  related  to 
presence  of  algae  might  induce  CA  activity  in  animal  tis- 
sue, such  as  increased  [O:]  or  decreased  [CO2]  due  to  pho- 


tosynthesis, changes  in  intracellular  pH  resulting  from  dif- 
ferent [CO;>],  or  products,  such  as  glycerol  or  amino  acids, 
translocated  from  alga  to  host  (see  Cook,  1983,  for  review). 
The  kinetics  ofCA  induction  ordeinduction  in  animal 
systems  has  been  studied  infrequently.  In  microalgae, 
however,  the  kinetics  of  CA  induction  are  well  described 
and,  in  all  cases,  are  shorter  in  duration  than  deinduction 
in  A.  pulchella.  Within  24  h  after  placing  C.  reinhardtii 
in  a  low  CO2  environment,  CA  activity  increased  up  to 
2000%  (Coleman  et  a/..  1984;  Badour  and  Tan,  1987). 
CA  has  even  been  reported  to  be  induced  and  deinduced 
at  the  transcriptional  level  on  a  diel  cycle  in  C.  reinhardtii 
when  the  chlorophyte  is  grown  in  12  h  light/dark  cycle 
(Toguri  et  ai,  1989). 

Other  examples  oj  induction  in  symbioses 

There  are  several  algal/cnidarian  symbioses  in  which 
algae  apparently  induce  enzyme  activity  or  developmental 
phenomena  in  the  animal.  For  example,  the  animal  tissue 


Table  III 

Relative  signal  strengths  of  the  two  polyckmal  antisera  used 
to  probe  the  experimental  samples 


ICN 

anti-human  CA 
1:1000  dilution 


BPS 

anti-human  CA 
1:200  dilution 


Purified  mammalian  CA 
Symbiotic  animal  tissue 
Cultured  zooxanthellae 


ICN  sheep  antiserum  was  purchased  from  ICN  and  BPS  sheep  anti- 
serum  from  Bioproducts  for  Science. 


CARBONIC  ANHYDRASE  IN  A  SEA  ANEMONE 


503 


of  the  symbiotic  anemone  Anthoplcura  elegantissima 
contains  high  levels  of  superoxide  dismutase  (SOD)  ac- 
tivity compared  with  SOD  in  nonsymbiotic  anemones. 
High  SOD  activity  is  interpreted  as  a  mechanism  for  re- 
moval of  damaging  superoxide  radicals  produced  during 
photosynthesis  by  the  symbiotic  algae  (Dykens  and  Shick, 
1982,  1984).  Also,  low  molecular  weight  fractions  from 
homogenates  of  symbiotic  cnidarians  suppress  uptake  of 
exogenous  alanine  by  isolated  zooxanthellae.  Similar 
fractions  from  aposymbiotic  animals  fail  to  suppress  up- 
take (Blanquet  ct  u/..  1988).  Metamorphosis  (which  in- 
volves complex  changes  in  enzyme  expression  and  activ- 
ity) of  scyphistomae  of  Cassiopeia  xanuichana  and  Mas- 
tigias  papua,  is  induced  by  the  presence  of  zooxanthellae 
(Sugiura,  1964;  Trench,  1979;  Hofmann  and  Kremer, 
1981). 

Enzyme  induction  has  also  been  demonstrated  in  other 
symbioses.  In  the  R/ii:ohinm/\egume  symbiosis,  the  bac- 
teroid  nitrogenase  activity  and  host  glutamate  synthetase 
activity  are  positively  correlated.  Further,  the  presence  of 
the  bacterial  symbionts  induces  the  synthesis  of  the  leghe- 
moglobin  apoprotein  (Smith  and  Douglas.  1987).  In  the 
bacteria/Amoeba  proteus  symbiosis,  peribacterial  vacuolar 
membranes  contain  a  protein  not  found  in  food  vacuolar 
membranes  (Jeon,  1983).  Jeon  suggests  that  the  synthesis 
of  this  protein  is  induced  by  the  presence  of  the  bacteria, 
and  that  the  protein  somehow  prevents  lysosomal  fusion 
with  the  peribacterial  vacuolar  membrane  and  subsequent 
digestion  of  the  bacteria. 

This  study  describes  another  example  of  genome  in- 
teraction between  two  partners  in  a  symbiosis.  Future 
studies  on  the  molecular  mechanisms  of  induction  and 
regulation  of  CA  should  prove  fruitful. 

Acknowledgments 

I  thank  S.  Anandan  and  M.  Harmon  for  valuable  tech- 
nical assistance.  R.  Gates.  M.  Harmon,  G.  Somero,  and 
L.  Muscatine  for  comments  of  the  manuscript,  and  L. 
Muscatine  for  valuable  suggestions  and  input.  This  study 
was  supported  by  a  research  grant  from  the  National  Sci- 
ence Foundation  (OCE-8510518  to  L.  Muscatine). 

Literature  Cited 

Badger,  M.  R.,  A.  Kaplan,  and  .).  A.  Berry.  1978.  A  mechanism  for 
concentrating  CO?  in  Chlamydomonas  reinluirdtii  and  Anabaenn 
variabilis  and  its  role  in  photosynthetic  CO;  fixation.  Carnegie  Inst. 
H  'axli.  Year  Book  77:  25 1  -26 1 . 

Badour,  S.  S.,  and  C.  K.  Tan.  1987.  Activities  of  carbonic  anhydrase 
and  carboxylating  enzymes  in  Chlamydomonas  segnis  adapted  and 
adapting  to  air  levels  of  carbon  dioxide.  Plant  Cell  Phvsioi  28(8): 
1485-1492. 

Blanquet,  R.  S.,  D.  Emmanuel,  and  T.  A.  Murphy.  1988.  Suppression 
of  exogenous  alanine  uptake  in  isolated  zooxanthellae  by  cnidarian 
host  homogenate  fractions:  species  and  symbiosis  specificity.  /  Exp. 
Mar  Biol.  Ecol.  117:  1-8. 


Coleman,  J.  K.  1980.  Current  concepts  of  the  mechanism  of  action  of 
carbonic  anhydrase.  Pp.  133-150  in  Biophysics  and  Physiology  ul 
Carbon  Dioxide  C.  Bauer.  G.  Gros,  and  H.  Bartels,  eds.  Springer 
Verlag,  Berlin. 

Coleman,  J.  R.,  J.  A.  Berry,  R.  K.  Togasaki,  and  A.  R.  Grossman. 
1984.  Identification  of  extracellular  carbonic  anhydrase  ofChluin- 
\\lnmonas  remhardlii  Plant  Phvsioi.  76:  472-477. 

Coleman,  J.  R.,  L.  S.  Green,  J.  A.  Berry,  R.  K.  Togasaki,  and  A.  R. 
Grossman.  1985.  Adaptation  of  Chlamydomonas  reinhardtii  to  air 
levels  of  CO;  and  the  induction  of  carbonic  anhydrase  activity.  Pp. 
339-359  in  Inorganic  Carbon  Uptake  by  Aquatic  Photosynthetic  Or- 
ganisms. W.  Lucas  and  J.  Berry,  eds.  Am.  Soc.  Plant  Pkysiol,.  Rock- 
vine,  MD. 

Cook,  C.  B.  1983.  Metabolic  interchange  in  algae-invertebrate  sym- 
bioses. //;/  Rev.  Cvlol.  14:  177-210. 

Deutsch,  H.  F.  1987.  Carbonic  anhydrases.  /;;/  J.  Biochcm  19(2):  101- 
113. 

Dionisio,  M.  L.,  M.  Tsuzuki,  and  S.  Miyachi.  1989a.  Light  requirement 
for  carbonic  anhydrase  induction  in  Chlamydomonas  reinhardtii. 
Plant  Cell  Phyxiol.  30(2):  207-213. 

Dionisio,  M.  L.,  M.  Tsuzuki,  and  S.  Miyachi.  1989b.  Blue  light  in- 
duction of  carbonic  anhydrase  activity  in  Chlamydomonas  reinhardtii. 
Plant  Cell  Phyxinl.  30(2):  215-219. 

Dixon,  G.  K.,  B.  N.  Patel,  and  M.  J.  Merrett.  1987.  Role  of  intracellular 
carbonic  anhydrase  in  inorganic-carbon  assimilation  by  Porphyndium 
purpureum  Planta  172:  508-513. 

Dykens,  J.  A.,  and  J.  M.  Shick.  1982.  Oxygen  production  by  endo- 
symbiotic  algae  controls  superoxide  dismutase  activity  in  their  animal 
host.  Nature  291:  579-580. 

Dykens,  J.  A.,  and  J.  M.  Shick.  1984.  Photobiology  of  the  symbiotic 
sea  anemone,  Anthopleura  elegantissima:  defenses  against  photo- 
dynamic  effects,  and  seasonal  photoacchmatization.  Biol.  Bull  167: 
683-697. 

Engvall.  E.  1980.  Enzyme  immunoassay  ELISA  and  EMIT.  Melli.  En- 
zvmol.  70:  419-439. 

Falkowski,  P.  G.,  Dubinsky,  Z.,  and  L.  Muscatine.  1984.  Light  and 
the  bioenergetics  of  a  symbiotic  coral.  BioSciencc  34:  705-709. 

Graham,  D.,  and  R.  M.  Smillie.  1976.  Carbonate  dehydratase  in  marine 
organisms  of  the  Great  Barrier  Reef.  Ami.  J.  Plant  Phvsioi.  3:  1 13- 
119. 

Graham,  D.,  M.  L.  Reed,  B.  D.  Patterson,  D.  G.  Hockley,  and  M.  R. 
Dwyer.  1984.  Chemical  properties,  distribution,  and  physiology  of 
plant  and  algal  carbonic  anhydrases.  Ann.  N.  Y.  Acad.  Set.  429:  222- 
237. 

Guillard,  R.  R.  L.,  and  M.  D.  Keller.  1984.  Culturing  dinoflagellates. 
Pp.  391-442  in  Dinoflagellates.  D.  L.  Spector,  ed.  Academic  Press. 
New  York. 

Hames,  B.  D.,  and  D.  Rickwood.  1987.  Gel  Eleclrophoresis  of  Proteins: 
A  Practical  Approach.  IRL  Press.  Oxford.  290  pp. 

Hartree,  E.  F.  1972.  Determination  of  protein:  a  modification  of  the 
Lowry  method  that  gives  a  linear  photometric  response.  Anal. 
Bioehem.  48:  422-427. 

Henry,  R.  P.  1988.  Multiple  functions  of  carbonic  anhydrase  in  the 
crustacean  gill.  /  Exp.  Zoo/.  248:  19-24. 

Henry,  R.  P.,  and  J.  N.  Cameron.  1983.  The  role  of  carbonic  anhydrase 
in  respiration,  ion  regulation  and  acid-base  balance  in  the  aquatic 
crab  Callinectes  sapidus  and  the  terrestrial  crab  Gecarcinus  lateralis. 
J.  Exp.  Biol.  103:  205-223. 

Hofmann,  D.  K.,  and  B.  P.  Kremer.  1981.  Carbon  metabolism  and 
strobilation  in  Cassiopea  andromedea  (Cnidaria:  Scyphozoa):  Sig- 
nificance of  endosymbiotic  dinoflagellates.  Mar.  Biol.  65:  25-33. 

Hogetsu,  D.,  and  S.  Miyachi.  1979.  Role  of  carbonic  anhydrase  in 
photosynthetic  CO,  fixation  in  Chlorella.  Plant  Cell  Phvsioi.  20(4): 
747-756. 


504 


V.  M.  WEIS 


Jeon,  K.  1983.  Integration  of  bacterial  endosymbionts  in  amoebae. 
1m.  Rev.  Cylol.  Suppl.  14:  29-47. 

Johnstone,  A.,  and  R.  Thorpe.  1987.  Immunocytochemistry  in  Practice. 
Blackwell  Scientific.  Oxford.  306  pp. 

Kerb),  N.  \\ .,  and  J.  A.  Raven.  1985.  Transport  and  fixation  of  in- 
organic carbon  by  marine  algae.  Adv.  Dot  Res  2:  71-123. 

Laemmli,  U.  K.  1970.  Cleavage  of  structural  proteins  during  the  as- 
sembly of  the  head  of  bacteriophage  T4.  Nature  227:  680-685. 

Lamb,  J.  E.  1977.  Minireview:  plant  carbonic  anhydrase.  LifeSci.  20: 
393-406. 

Lindskog,  S.,  L.  E.  Henderson,  K.  K.  Kannan,  A.  Liljas,  P.  O.  Nyman, 
and  B.Strandberg.  1971.  Carbonic  anhydrase.  Pp.  587-665  in  The 
En:ymcs.  I'olunw  5,  P.  Boyer,  ed.  Academic  Press.  New  York. 

Poincelot,  R.  P.  1979.  Carbonic  anhydrase.  Pp.  230-238  in  Encyclo- 
pedia oj  Plant  Physiology.  Photosynthesis  II  Photosynthetic  Carbon 
Metabolism  and  Related  Processes,  M.  Gibbs  and  E.  Latzko.  eds. 
Springer  Verlag,  Berlin. 

Reed,  M.  L.,  and  D.  Graham.  1981.  Carbonic  anhydrase  in  plants: 
distribution,  properties  and  possible  physiological  roles.  Prog.  Phy- 
tochem.  7:  47-94. 

Smith,  D.  C.,  and  A.  E.  Douglas.  1987.  Symbiosis  between  nitrogen- 
fixing  prokaryotes  and  plant  roots.  Pp  64-92  in  The  Biology  of  Sym- 
biosis. Edward  Arnold  Publishers,  London. 

Steen,  R.  G.,  and  L.  Muscatine.  1987.  Low  temperature  evokes  rapid 
exocytosis  of  symbiotic  algae  by  a  sea  anemone.  Biol.  Bull  172:  246- 
263. 

Sugiura,  V.  1964.  On  the  life  history  of  rhizostome  medusae  II.  Indis- 
pensability  of  zooxanthellae  for  strobilation  in  Mastigias  papua. 
Embriologia  8:  223-233. 


Tashian,  R.  E.  1989.  The  carbonic  anhydrases:  widening  perspectives 
on  their  evolution,  expression  and  function.  Bioessays  10(6):  186- 
192. 

Toguri,  T.,  S.  Muto,  S.  Mihara,  and  S.  Miyachi.  1989.  Synthesis  and 
degradation  of  carbonic  anhydrase  in  a  synchronized  culture  of 
Chlamydomonas  retnhardtii.  Plant  Cell  Physiol.  30(4):  533-539. 

lowbin,  H.,  T.  Staehelin,  and  J.  Gordon.  1979.  Electrophoretic  transfer 
of  proteins  from  polyacrvlamide  gels  to  nitrocellulose  sheets:  pro- 
cedure and  some  applications.  Proc.  Nat.  Acad.  Sci.  USA  76(9):  4350- 
4354. 

Trench,  R.  K.  1979.  The  cell  biology  ol  plant-animal  symbiosis.  Ann. 
Rev  Plant  Physiol.  30:  485-531. 

Tsuzuki,  M.,  Y.  Shiraiwa,  and  S.  Miyachi.  1980.  Role  of  carbonic 
anhydrase  in  photosynthesis  in  Chlorella  derived  from  kinetic  analysis 
of  14CO2  fixation.  Plant  Cell  Physiol  21(4):  677-688. 

Weis,  V.  M.,  G.  J.  Smith,  and  L.  Muscatine.  1989.  A  "CO;-supply" 
mechanism  in  zooxanthellate  cnidarians:  role  of  carbonic  anhydrase. 
Mar.  Biol.  100:  195-202. 

Wheatly,  M.  G.,  and  R.  P.  Henry.  1987.  Branchial  and  antennal  gland 
Na+/K+-dependent  ATPase  and  carbonic  anhydrase  activity  during 
salinity  acclimation  of  the  euryhaline  crayfish  Pacifastacus  leniusculus. 
J  E.\p  Biol  133:  73-86. 

\Vyeth,  P.,  and  R.  H.  Prince.  1977.  Carbonic  anhydrase.  Inorg.  Perspect. 
Biol  Med.  1:  37-71. 

Yagawa,  Y.,  S.  Muto,  and  S.  Miyachi.  1987.  Carbonic  anhydrase  of  a 
unicellular  red  alga  Porphyridiwn  cruenium  R-l.  I  Purification  and 
properties  of  the  enzyme.  Plant  Cell  Physiol  28(7):  1253-1262. 


Reference:  Bio/.  Bull  180:  505-507.  (June.  1941) 


Mitochondrial  Activities  of  Phosphagen  Kinases  are 
Not  Widely  Distributed  in  the  Invertebrates 

W.  ROSS  ELLINGTON1  AND  AMY  C.  MINES 

Department  of  Biological  Science.  B-157,  Florida  State  University.  Tallahassee.  Florida  32306 


A  diverse  array  ofphosphagen  kinases  [arginine  kinase 
(AK),  lombricine  kinase  (LK)],  glycocyamine  kinase  (GK). 
taurocyamine  kinase  (TK),  and  creatine  kinase  (CK)  is 
found  in  the  animal  kingdom  (see  ref.  1  for  a  review). 
These  reactions  appear  to  function  in  the  temporal  buff- 
ering of  ATP  in  muscles  during  energy  deficits  such  as 
might  occur  during  burst  contraction  or  anoxia  (2,  3).  In 
many  vertebrate  tissues,  a  distinct  mitochondria!  isoen- 
:yme  ofCK  is  present,  and  it  may  play  a  special  role  in 
the  intracellular  transport  of  high  energy  phosphate  (4). 
In  this  study,  we  investigated  whether  mitochondria!  ac- 
tivities ofphosphagen  kinases  are  present  in  invertebrate 
muscles.  Our  results  show  that  AK  is  present  in  mito- 
chondria from  a  crustacean.  However,  phosphagen  kinases 
are  lacking  in  mitochondria  from  insect  flight  muscles, 
molluscan  cardiac  and  smooth  muscle,  and polychaete  and 
oligochaete  body  wall  musculature.  It  appears  that  mito- 
chondria] activities  of  phosphagen  kinases  are  not  widely 
distributed  in  the  invertebrates.  These  data,  in  conjunction 
with  previous  studies  on  the  physico-chemical  nature  of 
the  interaction  ofphosphagen  kinases  with  mitochondria 
(5,  6),  suggest  that  mitochondrial  compartmentation  of 
phosphagen  kinases  may  have  evolved  independently  in 
two  major  animal  groups. 

Mitochondrial  CK  in  vertebrate  muscle  constitutes  the 
proximal  end  of  the  so-called  phosphocreatine  shuttle  (4). 
According  to  the  shuttle  model,  CK  catalyzes  the  phos- 
phorylation  of  creatine  to  phosphocreatine  using  newly 
synthesized  ATP.  The  resulting  phosphocreatine  is  then 
thought  to  diffuse  from  the  mitochondrion  to  sites  of  ATP 
use  (myofibrils,  ion  transport  ATPases),  where  it  is  used 

Received  9  October  1990;  accepted  3  March  1991. 
'  To  whom  editorial  correspondence  and  reprint  requests  should  he 
sent. 


to  phosphorylate  ADP  to  ATP.  In  effect,  high  energy 
phosphate  is  thought  to  be  transported  by  phosphocreatine 
rather  than  ATP,  which  overcomes  the  diffusion  limita- 
tions of  the  adenine  nucleotides  (4).  The  presence  of  mi- 
tochondrial CK  is  advantageous  because  it  maximizes  en- 
zymatic potential  in  the  compartment  where  it  is  needed 
(2).  In  contrast  to  the  situation  in  vertebrate  muscles,  we 
show  in  the  following  results  that  mitochondrial  activities 
of  other  phosphagen  kinases  are  rather  uncommon  in  the 
animal  kingdom. 

Tightly  coupled  mitochondria  were  isolated  from  the 
muscles  of  seven  representative  species  of  invertebrates, 
and  the  presence  of  phosphagen  kinase  activity  was  as- 
sessed by  respirometric  methods  (Table  I).  Phosphagen 
kinase  activities  were  not  present  in  mitochondria  from 
the  body  wall  musculature  of  the  earthworm  Lumbricus 
terrestris  and  the  polychaete  Nereis  virens.  Mitochondria 
from  the  radula  retractor  muscle  of  the  whelk  Busycon 
canalicitlatum  and  the  systemic  ventricle  of  the  octopus 
Octopus  vulgaris  lacked  AK  activity,  which  is  consistent 
with  results  from  studies  on  other  mollusks  (8,  9,  10).  AK 
was  also  not  present  in  mitochondria  from  the  flight  mus- 
cles of  the  blowfly  Sarcophaga  bullata  and  moth  Manduca 
sexta.  AK  also  appears  to  be  absent  from  the  flight  muscle 
mitochondria  of  the  locust  Locusta  migratoria  (11).  Spec- 
trophotometric  assays  (3)  ofphosphagen  kinase  activities 
in  detergent  extracts  of  L.  terrestris,  N.  virens,  and  M. 
sexta  mitochondria  revealed  only  trace  (<0. 1  ^mole/ 
min  •  g  wet  wgt  at  25°C),  or  no  activity. 

Only  mitochondria  isolated  from  the  hearts  of  the 
crayfish  Procambarus  clarkii  contained  phosphagen  ki- 
nase activity  (Table  I).  Mitochondrial  AK  activity  in  P. 
clarkii  was  sufficiently  high,  as  to  facilitate  stimulation  of 
approximately  50%  of  state- 3  respiration  when  5  mML- 
arginine  was  added  to  the  respiration  system  (Fig.  1 ).  Mi- 
tochondrial AK  represented  around  1 .5%  of  the  total  AK 


505 


506 


W.  R.  ELLINGTON  AND  A.  C.  MINES 


Table  I 


\liiin  honilru/l  activities  <>/  phospliiiKen  kinuses  in  the  muscles  oj  a 
viinet  i  nl  invertebrates.  A  "+"  or  "--"  nulictilcs  the  presence  or 
absence.  rcs/ici  'lively,  of  mitochondria!  kinase  activity.  Quality 
nl  iniinchnmlrnil  preparations  is  indicated  by  showing  the  range 
ul  values  i>l  the  respiratory  control  ratios  (RCR  =  State  3 
respiration  -H  State  4  respiration).  An  RCR  value  greater 
than  one  indicates  thai  mitochondria  are  coupled  and 
sluw  respiratory  control  behavior  in  response 
to  the  addition  ol  ADP  (see  discussion  below) 


Organism  and  tissue 


Phosphagen     Mitochondria] 

RCR          kinase  activity 


Lutnbricus  terrestris  body  wall 

(earthworm) 

3-4 

LK 

Nereis  virens  body  wall 

(polychaete) 

4-5 

GK 

Busycon  cana/iculatuin  radula 

muscle  (whelk) 

3-5 

AK 

Octopus  vulgaris  systemic 

ventricle  (octopus) 

8-17 

AK 

Sarcophaga  bullaia  flight 

muscle  (blowfly) 

4-7 

AK 

Manduca  se.\la  flight  muscle 

(moth) 

5-10 

AK 

Procambarus  clarkii  heart 

(crayfish) 

4-6 

AK                       + 

N.  virens  and  B  canalicitlatitin  were  obtained  from  the  Marine  Bio- 
logical Laboratory  (Woods  Hole.  Massachusetts).  L  terrestris.  S  bullata. 
and  M  sextawete  purchased  from  Carolina  Biological  Supply  (Burling- 
ton, North  Carolina).  O  vulgaris  and  P.  clarkii  were  collected  locally. 
Mitochondria  were  isolated  by  gentle  homogenization  and  differential 
centnfugation  procedures  (details  available  upon  request).  Mitochondnal 
respiration  was  monitored  polarigraphically  as  previously  described  (6. 
7).  The  addition  of  ADP  to  tightly  coupled  mitochondria  respiring  in 
the  presence  of  substrate  (state-4)  leads  to  a  dramatic  increase  in  respi- 
ration (state-3)  which  will  continue  until  all  of  the  ADP  has  been  phos- 
phorylated  to  ATP.  If  a  phosphagen  kinase  is  present  in  the  mitochondria, 
subsequent  addition  of  the  appropriate  phosphagen  acceptor  (arginine, 
lombncine,  glycocyamine,  etc.)  will  lead  to  the  formation  of  phosphagen 
and  ADP  by  the  following  reaction: 


Phosphagen  kinase  Acceptor  +  ATP 

Oxidative  phosphorylation     0O;  +  ADP  +  P, 

Net  reaction 


•  ADP  +  Phosphagen 

•  ATP  +  /}H,O 


/jOi  +  Acceptor  +  P,  ->  Phosphagen 


(Note:  0  is  dependent  on  the  P:O  ratio) 

The  resulting  ADP  will  stimulate  respiration  and  ATP  formation  via 
oxidative  phosphorylation  (see  above).  The  ATP  will  phosphorylate  ad- 
ditional acceptor,  producing  more  ADP  which  will  stimulate  state-3  res- 
piration as  long  as  acceptor  is  present  (net  reaction  above).  Thus,  stim- 
ulation of  state-3  respiration  by  phosphagen  acceptor  indicates  the  pres- 
ence of  mitochondria!  phosphagen  kinase  activity  (see  Fig.  1 .  for  example ). 
providing  that  the  mitochondria  have  been  extensively  washed,  as  was 
the  case  in  this  study.  Most  experiments  were  conducted  on  at  least  three 
independent  preparations  from  each  species.  Because  O  vulgaris  was 
not  readily  available,  only  a  single  mitochondrion  preparation  was  used 
for  the  experiments  with  this  species. 


activity  in  P  clarkii  heart  muscle  (Fig.  1).  AK  has  also 
been  observed  in  the  mitochondria  of  several  other  crus- 
taceans(12,  13,  14).  Furthermore,  we  have  recently  shown 
that  heart  mitochondria  from  the  horseshoe  crab  Limitlus 
polyphemus  (a  chelicerate  arthropod)  contain  AK  activity 
that  is  clearly  intrinsic  to  the  mitochondrion  (6,  7). 

Our  rather  limited  survey,  coupled  with  the  results  of 
others,  suggests  that  mitochondria!  phosphagen  kinase 
activities  are  consistently  present  in  the  muscles  of  only 
three  groups:  AK  in  crustaceans,  and  also  in  the  relic 
chelicerate  L.  polyphemus.  and  CK  in  vertebrates.  The 
interaction  between  AK  and  these  mitochondria  is  hy- 
drophobic,  in  that  detergents  are  required  to  solubilize 
enzyme  activity  (6,  14).  In  contrast,  the  interaction  be- 
tween CK  and  vertebrate  mitochondria  is  clearly  electro- 
static and  is  easily  disrupted  by  changes  in  ionic  strength 

Procambarus 


ATP 


,arg 


arg 


Figure  I.  Patterns  of  ox vgen  consumption  (vertical-oxygen  concentration  hor- 
i:onial-tnne)  of  mitochondria  from  the  heart  of  the  crayfish  Procambarus  clarkii. 
Mitochondria  were  added  to  an  isotonic  respiration  medium  supplemented  with  1.5 
mM  MgCI:  and  5  inM  potassium  phosphate,  and  respiration  was  monitored  at 
25°C.  Left  panel — 5  mM  a-keloglularate  (Kg)  was  initially  added  followed  hy  two 
cvcles  of  addition  of  200  jiM  ADP  After  return  to  state-4  respiralion.  5  mM  L- 
arninine  (arg)  was  added  lo  ascertain  whether  AK  was  present  Right  panel — Res- 
piration was  initiated  hy  the  addition  of  5  mM  a-keloglularate  followed  hy  200  ^M 
ATP.  Addition  of  5  mM  l.-argmint'  resulted  in  stimulation  ol  Slale-3  respiration 
via  ADP  production  hy  the  AK  reaction-  Respiration  was  further  enhanced  hy  addition 
0/200  iiM  ADP  In  both  sets  of  experiments,  L-argmine  was  capable  ol  stimulating 
respiratory  activity  equivalent  lo  approximately  50"f  ol  the  ADP-iiutiated  state-3 
rale.  To  verify  the  presence  ofAK  activity,  P.  clarkii  mitochondria  were  extracted 
in  detergent  (1%  Triton  X-100).  AK  activity  was  assayed  in  the  mitochondria!  extract 
using  prtTiouslv  described  spectrophototnelric  procedures  (7).  AK  activity  in  crayfish 
mitochondria  was  If*  iunnles/min  •  g  wet  wgt  at  25°C  which  represents  approximately 
1  5r('  at  [lit1  total  AK  activnv  in  this  tissue  Since  these  mitochondria  were  washed 
four  times,  it  is  clear  that  AK  activity  is  intrinsic  lo  P.  clarkii  mitochondria  and  is 
not  a  cytoplasmic  contaminant. 


MITOCHONDRIAL  ACTIVITIES  OF  PHOSPHAGEN  K.INASES 


507 


(5).  Given  the  broad  phylogenetic  distance  between  the 
crustacean  and  chelicerate  arthropods  and  the  vertebrates, 
the  apparent  lack  of  phosphagen  kinase  activities  in  the 
muscle  mitochondria  of  the  other  major  groups,  and  the 
dramatic  differences  in  the  physico-chemical  interaction 
between  these  kinases  and  the  mitochondria,  we  speculate 
that  mitochondrial  phosphagen  kinase  activities  arose  in- 
dependently in  the  two  groups  where  they  are  found. 

Finally,  we  point  out  that,  although  insect  flight  muscles 
and  cephalopod  hearts  develop  the  highest  aerobic  power 
outputs  of  any  invertebrate  muscles  (15,  16),  the  func- 
tional capabilities  of  these  muscles  do  not  appear  to  be 
intrinsically  limited  or  compromised  by  the  absence  of 
phosphagen  kinase  activities  in  their  mitochondria. 

Acknowledgments 

We  thank  J.  Otto  for  performing  some  of  the  initial 
mitochondrial  isolation  experiments  using  N.  vircns  tissue. 
Supported  by  NSF  Grant  DCB-8710108  to  WRE. 

Literature  Cited 

1.  Watts,  D.  C.  1971.     Evolution  of  phosphagen  kinases.  Pp.  150- 
1 73  in  Biochemical  Evolution  and  the  Origin  oj  Life.  E.  Schoffeniels, 
ed.  North  Holland  Publishing.  New  York. 

2.  Meyer,  R.  A.,  H.  L.  Sweeney,  and  M.  J.  Kushmerick.  1984.     A 
simple  analysis  of  the  phosphocreatine  shuttle.  Am.  J  1'hvxiol  246: 
C365-C377. 

3.  Ellington,  \V.  R.  1989.     Phosphocreatine  represents  a  thermody- 
namic  and  functional  improvement  over  other  muscle  phosphagens. 
/  Exp.  Biol  143:  177-194. 

4.  Bessman,  S.  P.,  and  P.  J.  Geiger.  1981.     Transport  of  energy  in 
muscle.  Science  211:  448-452. 

5.  Vial,    C.,    B.    Font,    D.    Goldschmidl,    and    D.    C.    Gauleron. 
1979.     Dissociation  and  reassociation  of  creatine  kinase  with  heart 


mitochondria:  pH  and  phosphate  dependence.  Bioclum.  Biophyx 
.Ir/n  88:  1352-1359. 

6.  Doumen.  C.,  and  \V.  R.  Ellington.  1990.     Mitochondrial  arginine 
kinase  from  the  heart  of  the  horseshoe  crab.  Limulits  polyphemus. 

I.  Physicochemical  properties  and  nature  of  interaction  with  the 
mitochondrion.  ./  Camp  Phyxiol.  B  160:  449-457. 

7.  Doumen,  C.  and  \V.  R.  Ellington.  1990.     Mitochondnal  arginine 
kinase  from  the  heart  of  the  horseshoe  crab,  Limii/iix  polyphemus. 

II.  Catalytic  properties  and  studies  of  potential  coupling  with  oxi- 
dative  phosphorylation.  ./  Comp.  Phyxiol.  B  160:  459-468. 

8.  Storey,  K.  B.  1977.     Purification  and  characterization  of  arginine 
kinase  from  the  mantle  muscle  of  the  squid.  Symplectoteuthis  oual- 
cinicnxix.  Arch  Biochem.  Biopliyx.  179:  518-526. 

9.  Ballantyne,   J.   S.,    P.    W.    Hochachka,   and   T.    P.    Mommsen. 
1981.     Studies  on  metabolism  of  the  migrating  squid  Loligo  opa- 
lexcens.  Enzymes  of  tissue  and  heart  mitochondria.  Afar.  Biol.  Lett. 
2:  75-85. 

10.  Mommsen,  T.  P.,  and  P.  VV.  Hochachka.  1981.     Respiratory  and 
enzymatic  properties  of  squid  heart  mitochondria.  Eur.  J.  Biochem 
120:  345-350. 

11.  Schneider,  A.,  R.  J.  Niener,  and  M.  K.  Grieshaber.  1989.     On  the 
role  of  arginine  kinase  in  insect  flight  muscle.  Insect  Biochem.  19: 
471-481). 

12.  Chen,  C.  H.,  and  A.  L.  Lehninger.  1973.     Respiration  and  phos- 
phorylation by  mitochondria  from  the  hepatopancreas  of  the  blue 
crab.  Arch.  Biochem.  Biophyx.  154:  449-459. 

13.  Skorkowski,  E.  E.,  Z.  Aleksandrowicz,  T.  Wrzolkowa,  and  J.  Swier- 
czyski.  1976.     Isolation  and  some  properties  of  mitochondria  from 
the  abdomen  muscle  of  the  crayfish  Orconectes  limoxus.  Cornp. 
Biochem  Phyxiol  56B:  493-500. 

14.  Hird,  E.  J.  R.,  and  V.  Robin.  1985.     Studies  on  phosphagen  synthesis 
by  mitochondrial  preparations.  Comp  Biochem  Phyxiol.  SOB:  5 1 7- 
520. 

15.  Ellington,  C.  P.  1985.     Power  and  efficiency  of  insect  flight  muscle. 
J.Exp.  Biol.  115:293-304. 

16.  Houlihan,  D.  I  ..  C.  Agnisda,  N.  M.  Hamilton,  and  I.  I  rara-Genoino. 
1985.     Oxygen  consumption  of  the  isolated  heart  of  Octopus  effects 
of  power  output  and  hypoxia.  /  Exp.  Biol.  131:  137-157. 


INDEX 


A  comparison  of  bursting  neurons  in  Aplysia.  269 

A  functional,  cellular,  and  evolutionary  model  of  nociceptive  plasticity 
in  Aplysia.  241 

Abalone,  3 1 8 

Abnormal  sea  urchin  fertilization  envelope  assembly  in  low  sodium  sea- 
water,  346 

Afferent  response  characteristics,  22 1 

Agonistic  behavior,  406 

ALAVSE.  A.  M..  see  J.  J.  Childress,  135 

Alcyoniiun  sideriwn.  81,  93 

ALEVIZOS,  A.,  M.  SKELTON,  K.  R.  WEISS,  AND  J.  K.OESTER,  A  com- 
parison of  bursting  neurons  in  Aplysia,  269 

ALEXANDER,  JAMES  E.,  JR.,  AND  ALAN  P.  COVICH,  Predation  risk  and 
avoidance  behavior  in  two  freshwater  snails,  387 

Alga.  112 

Algal/cnidanan  symbioses,  496 

Amino  acid  sequence,  485 

Analgesia.  301 

Aplysia.  252,  262,  269.  276 

Aplysia  eye,  284 

Appendicularia,  1 19 

Arousal,  262 

Anemia,  432 

Ascidian,  1 1 2 

Autotomy,  167 

Autotomy  in  blue  crab  (Callinectes  sapidux  Rathbun)  populations:  geo- 
graphic, temporal,  and  ontogenetic  variation.  416 

B 

Bag  cells,  269 

BARKER,  M.  F.,  see  M.  Byrne,  332 

BAXTER,  D.  A.,  see  L.  J.  Cleary.  252 

BELTZ,  B.  M.,  see  S.  M.  Helluy.  355 

Bicarbonate  use,  185 

Biological  clock,  284 

Biological  effects  of  magnetic  fields.  30 1 

Bioluminescence,  440 

Bivalve  mollusks,  466 

Bivalve  veligers,  103 

BLACKSTONE,  NEIL  W.,  AND  LEO  Buss,  Shape  variation  in  hydractiniid 
hydroids,  394 

Blue  crabs,  447 

BOLLNER,  TOMAS,  JON  STORM-MATHISEN,  AND  OLE  PETTER  OTTER- 
SEN,  GABA-like  immunoreactivity  in  the  nervous  system  of  Oi- 
kopleura  dioica  (Appendicularia).  1 19 

BOWLBY.  MARK  R..  AND  JAMES  F.  CASE,  Ultrastructural  and  neuronal 
control  of  luminous  cells  in  the  copepod  Guussia  princeps,  440 

Brine  shrimp,  432 

Brittlestars,  167 

BROLIWER.  MARIUS,  see  David  W.  Engel,  447 

Bryozoa.  1  1 2 

Bursting  neurons,  269 

Buss,  LEO,  see  Neil  W.  Blackstone,  394 

BYRNE,  J.  H.,  see  L.  J.  Cleary.  252 

BYRNE,  M.,  AND  M.  F.  BARKER,  Embryogenesis  and  larval  development 
of  the  asteroid  Patiriella  regu/aris  viewed  by  light  and  scanning 
electron  miscroscopy,  332 


Calcification,  185,489 

Calcitonin,  485 

Calcium  ATPase.  185 

Calcium-proton  exchange  during  algal  calcification,  185 

C  'allinccles  sapidus.  4 1 6 

CAMP.  252 

Cancer.  125 

Carbon  budgets  for  two  species  of  benthonic  symbiont-bearing  Fora- 

minifera.  489 
Carbonic  anhydrase,  496 
Carcinus  maenas  larvae,  65 
CARGO,  DAVID  G.,  see  Jennifer  E.  Purcell,  103 
CARLTON,  DEBBY  A.,  see  James  T.  Carlton,  72 
CARLTON,  JAMES  T.,  GEERAT  J.  VERMEIJ,  DAVID  R.  LINDBERG,  DEBBY 

A.  CARLTON,  AND  ELIZABETH  C.  DUDLEY,  The  first  historical  ex- 
tinction of  a  marine  invertebrate  in  an  ocean  basin:  the  demise  of 

the  eelgrass  limpet  Lollia  alveus,  72 
Cartilaginous  fish,  485 
CASE,  JAMES  F.,  see  Mark  R.  Bowlby.  440 
Catecholamines,  310 
cDNA  sequences  reveal  mRNAs  for  two  G«  signal  transducing  proteins 

from  larval  cilia,  318 
Cepaca  nemnralis,  30 1 
Cephalopod  swimming,  221 
CHANG,  ERNEST  S.,  see  Mark  J.  Snyder,  475 
Chara  corallina.  185 
CHARMANTIER,  G.,  AND  M.  CHARMANTIER-DAURES.  Otogeny  of  os- 

moregulation  and  salinity  tolerance  in  Cancer  irrnraliis:  elements 

of  comparison  with  C.  borealis  (Crustacea,  Decapoda),  125 
CHARMANTIER-DAURES,  M.,  see  G.  Charmantier,  125 
Chemical  mediation  of  larval  release  behaviors  in  the  crab  Neopanope 

sayi.  1 

Chemosensory,  318 
CHENG,  Sou-DE,  PATRICIA  S.  GLAS,  AND  JEFFREY  D.  GREEN.  Abnormal 

sea  urchin  fertilization  envelope  assembly  in  low  sodium  seawater. 

346 
CHILDRESS,  J.  J..  C.  R.  FISHER,  J.  A.  FAVUZZI,  R.  E.  KOCHEVAR. 

N.  K.  SANDERS,  AND  A.  M.  ALAYSE,  Sulfide-driven  autotrophic 

balance  in  the  bacterial  symbiont-containing  hydrothermal  vent 

tubeworm.  Rijiia  pachyptila  Jones,  135 
Chrysaora  quinquercirrha,  103 
Cilia,  12,  318 
Circadian  pacemaker.  284 
CLEARY,  L.  J.,  D.  A.  BAXTER.  F.  NAZIF,  AND  J.  H.  BYRNE,  Neural 

mechanisms  underlying  sensitization  of  a  defensive  reflex  in  Aplysia. 

252 

CLEMENTS,  LEE  ANN.  see  William  E.  Dobson,  167 
Clione  limacina.  228 
Cloning,  318 

Colonial  invertebrates.  112 
Command  motivation,  262 
Competition,  394,  406 

Computation  in  the  learning  system  of  cephalopods.  200 
Contraction,  serotonin-elicited  modulation,  and  membrane  currents  of 

dissociated  fibers  of  Aplysia  buccal  muscle,  276 
Control  of  central  and  peripheral  targets  by  a  multifunctional  peptidergic 

interneuron,  295 
Copepod,  440 


508 


INDEX  TO  VOLUME   180 


509 


Copper,  447 

Corallimorphanan,  406 

COVICH,  ALAN  P.,  see  James  E.  Alexander,  387 

Crab  larval  release  behaviors,  1 

Crassostrea  virginica,  103 

Crepidu/a.  372 

CRESSWELL,  FRANCES  P.,  see  Jennifer  E.  Purcell.  103 

CROWE,  JOHN  H.,  see  Laurie  E.  Dnnkwater,  432 

Crustacea,  125,  154 

Crustacean  development,  355 

Ctenophore,  103 

Cysts.  432 


D 


Diixyalix  akajci.  485 

Day-night  rhythms,  301 

DE  VRIES,  M.  C..  D.  RITTSCHOF,  AND  R.  B.  FORWARD,  JR.,  Chemical 
mediation  of  larval  release  behaviors  in  the  crab  Neopanope  sari.  1 

Defensive  behavior,  24 1 

Development,  209,  372 

Development  of  giant  motor  axons  and  neural  control  of  escape  responses 
in  squid  embryos  and  hatchlings.  209 

DiCksoN,  JOHN  S.,  RICHARD  M.  DILLAMAN,  ROBERT  D.  ROER,  AND 
DAVID  B.  ROVE,  Distribution  and  characterization  of  ion  trans- 
porting and  respiratory  filaments  in  the  gills  of  Procamharus  cliirkii, 
154 

DIETZ.  THOMAS  H.,  see  David  B.  Gardiner.  453 

Differential  ingcstion  and  digestion  of  bivalve  larvae  by  the  scyphozoan 
Chrvasora  quinquccirrha  and  the  Ctenophore  Mnemiopsis  leiityi. 
103" 

Digestion.  103 

DILLAMAN,  RICHARD  M.,  see  John  S.  Dickson,  154 

DIRCKSEN.  H..  see  S.  G.  Webster,  65 

Discocilia.  466 

Dispersal.  34 

Distribution  and  characterization  of  ion  transporting  and  respiratory 
filaments  in  the  gills  of  Procambarus  clarkii,  1 54 

DOBSON,  WILLIAM  E..  STEPHEN  E.  STANCYK.  LEE  ANN  CLEMENTS. 
AND  RICHARD  M.  SHOWMAN.  Nutrient  translocation  during  early 
disc  regeneration  in  the  bnttlestar  Microphiopholis  gracillima 
(Stimpson)  (Echmodermata:  Ophiuroidea).  167 

DRINKWATER,  LAURIE  E..  AND  JOHN  H.  CROWE.  Hydration  state,  me- 
tabolism, and  hatching  of  Mono  Lake  Anemia  cysts,  432 

DUDLEY,  ELIZABETH  C.,  see  James  T.  Carlton.  72 


E 


Ecdysis,  447 

Ecdysone,  475 

Ecdysteroids.  475 

Echinodermata.  12,  167 

Eelgrass,  72 

Electromagnetic  fields,  301 

Electroretinogram.  284 

ELLINGTON.  W.  Ross,  AND  AMY  C.  HINES,  Mitochondrial  activities  of 

phosphagen  kinases  are  not  widely  distributed  in  the  invertebrates, 

505 
Embryogenesis  and  larval  development  of  the  asteroid  Patiriella  regulwis 

viewed  by  light  and  scanning  electron  miscroscopy,  332 
Embryonic  development  of  the  American  lobster  (Homanis  americanus): 

quantitative  staging  and  characterization  of  an  embryonic  molt  cycle, 

355 
ENGEL.  DAVID  W.,  AND  MARILJS  BROUWER,  Short-term  methallothi- 

onein  and  copper  changes  in  blue  crabs  at  ecdysis.  447 
Enzyme  induction.  496 
EREZ.  J..  see  B.  H.  ter  Kiule,  489 
Escape 

behavior,  209 
swimming.  228 


Expansion  of  the  sperm  nucleus  and  association  of  the  maternal  and 

paternal  genomes  in  fertilized  Mulinia  /a/era/is  eggs,  56 
Extinction,  72 


Factors  affecting  the  sensory  response  characteristics  of  the  cephalopod 
statocyst  and  their  relevance  in  predicting  swimming  performance, 
221 

FAIR.  RICHARD  H.,  see  Ted  A.  McConnaughey,  185 
FAVUZZI.  J.  A.,  see  J.  J.  Childress,  135 
Feeding.  12.  103 

efficiency,  93 

rate.  8 1 
Fertilization. 

envelopes.  346 

in  Mulinia,  56 

effects  of  UV  irradiation,  56 
FISHER,  C.  R.,  see  J.  J.  Childress,  135 
Flow,  93 

Foraminifera,  larger.  489 
FORWARD,  R.  B.,  see  M.  C.  De  Vnes,  I 
Freshwater  mussels,  453 


G-protein.  3 1 8 

GABA-like  immunoreactivity  in  the  nervous  system  ofOikopleura  dioica 

(Appendicularia).  119 
GARDINER,  DAVID  B.,  HAROLD  SILVERMAN,  AND  THOMAS  H.  DIETZ, 

Musculature  associated  with  the  water  canals  in  freshwater  mussels 

and  response  to  monoammes  in  vitro,  453 
Gastropod  development.  372 
Gastropod  egg  capsules  and  their  contents  from  deep-sea  hydrothermal 

vent  environments,  34 
GEE,  CHRISTINE,  see  C.  K.  Govind,  28 
Genetic  variation,  394 
Geographic  and  temporal  variation,  416 
Giant  axons,  209 
Giant  neurons.  234 
Gill.  154,453 
Gill  musculature,  453 

GILLETTE,  RHANOR,  On  the  significance  of  neuronal  giantism  in  gas- 
tropods, 234 
GILLY,  W.  F.,  BRUCE  HOPKINS,  AND  G.  O.  MACK.IE,  Development  of 

giant  motor  axons  and  neural  control  of  escape  responses  in  squid 

embryos  and  hatchlings.  209 
GLAS,  PATRICIA  S.,  see  Sou-De  Cheng,  346 
GOVIND.  C.  K...  CHRISTINE  GEE.  AND  JOANNE  PEARCE,  Retarded  and 

mosaic  phenotype  in  regenerated  claw  closer  muscles  of  juvenile 

lobsters.  28 

GREEN,  JEFFREY  D.,  see  Sou-De  Cheng,  346 
GUSTAFSON.  R.  G..  D.  T.  J.  LITTLEWOOD.  AND  R.  A.  LuTZ,  Gastropod 

egg  capsules  and  their  contents  from  deep-sea  hydrothermal  vent 

environments,  34 


H 

HADFIELD,  MICHAEL  G..  see  Anthony  Pires,  310 

Halioiis.  318 

Handling  times,  387 

HANLON,  ROGER  T.,  Integrative  neurobiology  and  behavior  of  mollusks 
symposium:  introduction,  perspectives,  and  round-table  discussions, 
197 

HART,  MICHAEL  W.,  Particle  captures  and  the  method  of  suspension 
feeding  by  ehinoderm  larvae,  12 

HELLUY,  S.  M.,  AND  B.  S.  BELTZ,  Embryonic  development  of  the  Amer- 
ican lobster  (Homanis  americanus):  quantitative  staging  and  char- 
acterization of  an  embryonic  molt  cycle,  355 

Heterochrony,  394 

HINES,  AMY  C.,  see  W.  Ross  Ellington,  505 


510 


INDEX  TO  VOLUME   180 


HINES,  ANSON  H.,  see  L.  David  Smith,  416 

Homurus.  329,  355 

HOPKINS,  BRUCE,  see  W.  F.  Gilly.  209 

How  do  temperature  and  salinity  affect  relative  rates  of  growth,  mor- 
phological differentiation,  and  time  to  metaphoric  competence  in 
larvae  of  the  marine  gastropod  Crepidula  piano!.  372 

Hydractinia,  394 

Hydration  state,  metabolism,  and  hatching  of  Mono  Lake  Anemia  cysts, 
432 

Hydrogen  peroxide,  310 

Hydroid,  394 

Hydrothermal  vent,  34,  135 

I 

Immunocytochemistry,  65 

Inducible  agonistic  structures  in  the  tropical  corallimorpharian.  Disco- 
soma  sanctithomae,  406 

Inducible  structures,  406 

Inhibition,  241 

Injury.  241 

Integrative  neurobiology  and  behavior  of  mollusks  symposium:  intro- 
duction, perspectives,  and  round-table  discussions,  197 

Ion  transport.  154 


JACKLET,  JON  W.,  Photoresponsiveness  of  Aplysia  eye  is  modulated  by 
the  ocular  Orcadian  pacemaker  and  serotonin.  284 


K 

KAVALIERS,  MARTIN,  AND  KLAUS-PETER  OSSENKOPP,  Opiod  systems 
and  magnetic  field  effects  in  the  land  snail,  Ct'paea  nemoralis,  301 
KENNEDY,  VICTOR  S.,  see  Jennifer  E.  Purcell.  103 
KOCHEVAR.  R.  E.,  see  J.  J.  Childress.  135 
KOESTER.  J..  see  A.  Alevizos,  269 
KRAVITZ.  EDWARD  A.,  The  rime  of  the  ancient  scientist,  329 

KUPFERMANN.    IRVING,    THOMAS    TEYKE,    STEVEN    C.    ROSEN,    AND 

KLAUDIUSZ  R.  WEISS,  Studies  of  behavioral  state  in  Aplysia.  262 


Larvaceaus  urochordate,  119 
Larvae.  12.  318.  372 
Larval 

development.  332 

settlement.  1 1 2 
Limpets,  72 

LINDBERG,  DAVID  R.,  see  James  T.  Carlton,  72 
LITTLEWOOD,  D.  T.  J.,  see  R.  G.  Gustafson,  34 
Liu,  Li-XiN,  see  Jeffrey  L.  Ram,  276 
Lobster,  28,  329,  355,  475 
Locomotion.  228 

LONGO,  FRANK.,  JR.,  AND  JOHN  SCARPA,  Expansion  of  the  sperm  nucleus 
and  association  of  the  maternal  and  paternal  genomes  in  fertilized 
Mulinia  laleralis  eggs,  56 
Lot tiu  ulreus,  12 
Luminous  cell,  440 
LUTZ.  R.  A.,  see  R.  G.  Gustafson.  34 


M 

MACKIE,  G.  O..  see  W.  F.  Gilly.  209 

Magnetic  fields.  30 1 

Marine.  I  12 

Mathematical  model,  81 

McCONNAUGHEY,  TED  A.,  AND  RICHARD  H.  FALK,  Calcium-proton 

exchange  during  algal  calcification.  185 
Mechanosensory  neuron,  241 


Meiotic  maturation 

relationship  to  sperm  nuclear  transformation,  56 

in  Mulinia,  56 
Memory.  241 
Messenger  RNA.  318 
Metabolism  and  excretion  of  injected  ['H]-ecdysone  by  female  lobsters. 

Homurus  americantis,  475 
Metamorphosis,  125.  310.  372 
Methallothionein,  447 
MILES.  J.  S.,  Inducible  agonistic  structures  in  the  tropical  corallimor- 

phanan.  Discosoma  sanctithomae,  406 
Mitochondria!  activities  of  phosphagen  kinases  are  not  widely  distributed 

in  the  invertebrates,  505 
Mnemiopsis  leidyi,  103 
Modulation.  228 
Mollusk,  228.  234.  301.  318 
Molt  cycle.  355 

Molt-inhibiting  hormone  (MIH),  65 
Molting.  475 
Morphogenesis,  310 
Morphology,  252,  394 

MORSE,  DANIEL  E.,  see  Lisa  M.  Wodicka,  318 
Motor  control,  209 
Mulinia 

fertilization,  56 

meiotic  maturation.  56 

sperm  nuclear  transformation,  56 

pronuclear  development,  56 
Muscle.  28.  276 

Musculature  associated  with  the  water  canals  in  freshwater  mussels  and 
response  to  monoamines  in  vitro,  453 

N 

NAKAJIMA,  K.,  see  Y.  Takei.  485 

NAZIF,  F.,  see  L.  J.  deary,  252 

Neural  control  of  speed  changes  in  an  opisthobranch  locomotory  system, 

228 
Neural  mechanisms  underlying  sensitization  of  a  defensive  reflex  in 

Aplysia.  252 

New  calcitonin  isolated  from  the  ray.  Dasyatis  akajci.  485 
Nociception.  301 
Nociceptor  regeneration.  241 
Nudibranch,  310 
Nutrient  translocation  during  early  disc  regeneration  in  the  brittlestar 

Miarophiopholis  gracillima  (Stimpson)  (Echmodermata:  Ophiu- 

roidea),  167 

O 

Octopus  memory.  200 

OGURO.  C..  see  V.  Takei,  485 

Oikopleura.  1 19 

On  the  nature  of  paddle  cilia  and  discolia,  466 

On  the  significance  of  neuronal  giantism  in  gastropods.  234 

Ontogenetic  differences,  4 1 6 

Ontongeny  of  osmoregulation,  125 

Opiates,  301 

Opiod  systems  and  magnetic  field  effects  in  the  land  snail,  Cepaea  ne- 
moralis. 301 

Opisthobranch,  228.  310 

Orientation.  262 

OSSENKOPP,  KLAUS-PETER,  see  Margin  Kavaliers.  301 

Ostia  serotonin.  453 

Ontogeny  of  osmoregulation  and  salinity  tolerance  in  Cancer  irroratus; 
elements  of  comparison  with  C.  borealis  (Crustacea,  Decapoda). 
125 

OTTERSEN,  OLE  PETTER,  see  Tomas  Bollner,  1 19 

Ovoperoxides.  346 

Oxidative  breakdown  products  of  catecholamines  and  hydrogen  peroxide 
induce  partial  metamorphosis  in  the  nudibranch  Phcslilla  sibogoe 
Bergh  (Gastropoda:  Opisthobranchia).  310 

Oyster.  103 


INDEX  TO  VOLUME   180 


511 


Paddle  cilia,  466 

Pain  and  analgesia,  241 

Partial  predation.  416 

Particle  captures  and  the  method  of  suspension  feeding  by  echinoderm 
larvae,  12 

Passive  suspension  feeding,  81,  93 

Passive  suspension  feeding  by  an  octocoral  in  plankton  patches:  empirical 
tests  of  a  mathematical  model,  8 1 

Patirii'lla.  332 

PATTERSON.  MARK  R..  Passive  suspension  feeding  by  an  octocoral  in 
plankton  patches:  empirical  tests  of  a  mathematical  model.  81 

PATTERSON.  MARK  R.,  The  effects  of  flow  on  polyp-level  prey  capture 
in  an  octocoral,  Alcyonium  siderium,  93 

PEARCE,  JOANNA,  see  C.  K.  Govind.  28 

PECHENIK,  JAN  A.,  see  Kerry  M.  Zimmerman,  372 

pH  bonding,  185 

Phosphagen  kinase,  505 

Photoresponsiveness  ofAplyxia  eye  is  modulated  by  the  ocular  circadian 
pacemaker  and  serotonin,  284 

Phywlla.  387 

Physiology,  167 

PIRES,  ANTHONY,  AND  MICHAEL  G.  HADFIELD,  Oxidative  breakdown 
products  of  catecholamines  and  hydrogen  peroxide  induce  partial 
metamorphosis  in  the  nudibranch  Plicxtilla  xihogoe  Bergh  (Gastro- 
poda: Opisthobranchia),  310 

Plankton  patches,  81 

Planorbella,  387 

Podocryne.  394 

Polyp  feeding,  93 

Polyploidy  nervous  system  evolution,  234 

Potassium  channels.  252 

Predation,  103,  387 

Predation  risk  and  avoidance  behavior  in  two  freshwater  snails,  387 

Predator  avoidance,  387 

Prey  capture,  93 

PRIOR,  DAVID  J..  Control  of  central  and  peripheral  targets  by  a  multi- 
functional peptidergic  interneuron.  295 

Procambarus,  387 

Procambanis  clarkii.  1 54 

Proton  exchange,  185 

PURCELL,  JENNIFER  E.,  FRANCIS  P.  CRESSWELL,  DAVID  G.  CARGO, 
AND  VICTOR  S.  KENNEDY,  Differential  ingestion  and  digestion  of 
bivalve  larvae  by  the  scyphozoan  Chryasora  quinquecirrha  and  the 
ctenophore  Mnemiopsis  leidyi,  103 

Putative  molt-inducing  hormone  in  larvae  of  the  shore  carb  Carcinus 
maenas  L.:  an  immunocytochemical  approach,  65 


RAM,  JEFFREY  L.,  FENG  ZHANG,  AND  Li-XlN  Liu,  Contraction,  sero- 
tonin-elicited modulation,  and  membrane  currents  of  dissociated 
fibers  of  Aplvsia  buccal  muscle,  276 

Refuges.  112 

Regeneration.  28,  167 

Respiration,  154 

Respiratory  pumping.  269 

Retarded  and  mosaic  phenotype  in  regenerated  claw  closer  muscles  of 
juvenile  lobsters.  28 

Riftia,  135 

RITTSCHOF,  D.,  see  M.  C.  De  Vries,  1 

ROER,  ROBERT  D.,  see  John  S.  Dp  kson,  154 

ROSEN,  STEVEN  C.,  see  Irving  Kupfermann,  262 

ROYE,  DAVID  B.,  see  John  S.  Dickson,  154 


SATTERLIE.  RICHARD  A.,  Neural  control  of  speed  changes  in  an  opis- 

thobranch  locomotory  system.  228 
SCARPA.  JOHN,  see  Frank  Longo.  Jr..  56 
Scyphomedusae.  103 
Sea  urchin  fertilization,  346 
Sensitization,  241,  252 
Sensory  neurons,  252 
Serotonin,  252.  276,  284 
Settlement,  refuges,  and  adult  body  form  in  colonial  marine  invertebrates: 

a  field  experiment,  1 12 
Shape  variation  in  hydractiniid  hydroids,  394 
SHORT,  GRAHAM,  AND  SIDNEY  L.  TAMM.  On  the  nature  of  paddle  cilia 

and  discolia,  466 
Short-term  methallothionein  and  copper  changes  in  blue  crabs  at  ecdysis, 

447 

SHOWMAN,  RICHARD  M.,  see  William  E.  Dobson,  167 
Signal  transduction,  318 

SILVERMAN,  HAROLD,  see  David  B.  Gardiner,  453 
SKELTON,  M.,  see  A.  Alevizos,  269 
SMITH.  L  DAVID,  AND  ANSON  H.  MINES,  Autotomy  in  blue  crab  (Cal- 

linectes  sapitlux  Rathbun)  populations:  geographic,  temporal,  and 

ontogenetic  variation.  416 
Smooth  muscle,  276 
Snail.  301 
SNYDER,  MARK  J.,  AND  ERNEST  S.  CHANG,  Metabolism  and  excretion 

of  injected  [-'H]-ecdysone  by  female  lobsters,  Homarus  americamis, 

475 

Sodium  dependency,  346 
Squid,  209 
Staging.  355 

STANCYK,  STEPHEN  E.,  see  William  E.  Dobson,  167 
Statocyst  function,  221 

STORM-MATHISEN  JON,  see  Tomas  Bollner,  1 19 
Stretch-activated  channels,  276 
Stronglycentrotus  purpuraiits,  346 
Sulfide-driven  autotrophic  balance  in  the  bacterial  symbiont-containing 

hydrothermal  vent  tubeworm,  Riftia  pachyptila  Jones,  135 
Surface  topography,  1 1 2 
SUZUKI.  N.,  see  Y.  Takei,  485 
Swimming.  228 
Symbiosis.  135.489,496 


TAKAHASHI,  A.,  see  Y.  Takei,  485 

TAKEI,  Y.,  A.  TAKAHASHI,  T.  X.  WATANABE.  K.  NAKAJIMA,  S.  SAK- 

AKIBARA,  Y.  SASSAYAMA.  N.  SUZUKI,  AND  C.  OGURO,  New  cal- 

citonin  isolated  from  the  ray,  Daxyatix  akajci.  485 
TAMM,  SIDNEY  L.,  see  Graham  Short,  466 
TER  KUILE,  B.  H.,  AND  J.  EREZ.  Carbon  budgets  for  two  species  of 

benthonic  symbiont-beanng  Foraminifera,  489 
TEYKE,  THOMAS,  see  Irving  Kupfermann,  262 
The  effects  of  flow  on  polyp-level  prey  capture  in  an  octocoral,  Alcyonium 

siderium.  93 
The  first  historical  extinction  of  a  marine  invertebrae  in  an  ocean  basin: 

the  demise  of  the  eelgrass  limpet  Lttllia  alveus.  72 
The  induction  of  carbonic  anhydrase  in  the  symbiotic  sea  anemone  Aip- 

taxia  piilclwllu,  496 
The  rime  of  the  ancient  scientist,  329 
Transepithehal  potential,  154 
Tubeworm,  135 
Twilight,  301 


u 


SAKAKIBARA.  S..  see  Y.  Takei,  485 
Salinity  tolerance,  125,432 
SANDERS,  N.  K.,  see  J.  J.  Childress,  135 
SASAYAMA,  Y.,  see  Y.  Takei,  485 


Ultrastructure,  1 54,  440 

Ultrastructure  and  neuronal  control  of  luminous  cells  in  the  copepod 

Gaussia  princepx.  440 
Unfreezable  water,  432 
Unionid  gill,  453 


512 


INDEX  TO  VOLUME   180 


Veliger,  310.466 

VERMEU.  GEERAT  J.,  see  James  T.  Carlton.  72 

Vestibular  system.  22 1 

Voltage-dependent  calcium  channel  neuromodulation.  276 

Vulnerability,  387 

w 

WALTERS,  EDGAR  T.,  A  functional,  cellular,  and  evolutionary  model  of 

nociceptive  plasticity  in  Aplysis,  24 1 
WALTERS,  LINDA  J.,  AND  DAVID  S.  WETHEV,  Settlement,  refuges,  and 

adult  body  form  in  colonial  marine  interebrates:  a  field  experiment, 

112 

WATANABE,  T.  X.,  See  Takei.  485 
Water  canals.  453 
Waterflow,  453 
WEBSTER.  S.  G.,  AND  H.  DIRCKSEN,  Putative  molt-inducing  hormone 

in  larvae  of  the  short  crab  Carcinus  maenas  L.:  an  immunocyto- 

chemical  approach,  65 
WEIS,  VIRGINIA  M.  The  induction  of  carbonic  anhydrase  in  the  symbiotic 

sea  anemone  Aipaslia  ptilchella.  496 


WEISS.  K.  R.,  see  A.  Alevizo,  269,  and  Irving  Kupfermann.  262 

WETHEV,  DAVID  S.,  see  Linda  J.  Walters.  1 12 

Williamson.  Roddy.,  Factors  affecting  the  sensory  response  characteristics 
of  the  cephalopod  statocyst  and  their  relevance  in  predicting  swim- 
ming performance,  221 

WODICKA.  LISA  M.,  AND  DANIEL  E.  MORSE.  cDNA  sequences  reveal 
mRNAs  for  two  G«  signal  transducing  proteins  from  larval  cilia. 
318 

Wound-healing,  167 


YOUNG,  J.  Z..  Computation  in  the  learning  system  of  cephalopods,  200 


ZHANG.  FENG,  see  Jeffrey  L.  Ram,  276 

ZIMMERMAN,  KERRY  M.,  AND  JAN  PECHENIK,  How  do  temperature 
and  salinity  affect  relative  rates  of  growth,  morphological  differ- 
entiation, and  time  to  metaphoric  competence  in  larvae  of  the  ma- 
rine gastropod  Cn'piihila  pinna'.'  372 


36(46    D35 


CONTENTS 


Kravitz,  Edward  A. 

The  rime  of  the  ancient  scientist 


Hydration  state,  metabolism,  and  hatching  of  Mono 
329  Lake  Artemia  cysts    432 


DEVELOPMENT  AND  REPRODUCTION 

Byrne,  M.,  and  M.  F.  Barker 

Embryogenesis  and  larval  development  of  the  as- 
teroid Patiriella  regularis  viewed  by  light  and  scan- 
ning electron  microscopy 332 

Cheng,  Sou- I)c,  Patricia  S.  Glas,  and  Jeffrey  D.  Green 
Abnormal  sea  urchin  fertilization  envelope  assembly 
in  low  sodium  seawater  346 

Helluy,  S.  M .,  and  B.  S.  Beltz 

Embryonic  development  of  the  American  lobster 
(Homarus  americanus):  quantitative  staging  and 
characterization  of  an  embryonic  molt  cycle  ....  355 

Zimmerman,  Kerry  M.,  and  Jan  A.  Pechenik 

How  do  temperature  and  salinity  affect  relative  rates 
of  growth,  morphological  differentiation,  and  time 
to  metamorphic  competence  in  larvae  of  the  marine 
gastropod  Crepidula  plana?  372 

ECOLOGY  AND  EVOLUTION 

Alexander,  James  E.,  Jr.,  and  Alan  P.  Covich 

Predation  risk  and  avoidance  behavior  in  two  fresh- 
water snails  387 

Blackstone,  Neil  W.,  and  Leo  W.  Buss 

Shape  variation  in  hydractiniid  hydroids    394 

Miles,  J.  S. 

Inducible  agonistic  structures  in  the  tropical  coral- 
limorpharian,  Discosoma  sanctithomae  406 

Smith,  L.  David,  and  Anson  H.  Hines 

Autotomy  in  blue  crab  (Callinecles  sapidus  Rathbun) 
populations:  geographic,  temporal,  and  ontogenetic 
variation  416 

ENVIRONMENTAL  PHYSIOLOGY 
Drinkwater,  Laurie  E.,  and  John  H.  Crowe 


PHYSIOLOGY 

Bowlby,  Mark  R .,  and  James  F.  Case 

Ultrastructure  and  neuronal  control  of  luminous 
cells  in  the  copepod  Gaussia  princeps  440 

Engel,  David  W.,  and  Marius  Brouwer 

Short-term  metallothionein  and  copper  changes  in 
blue  crabs  at  ecdysis 447 

Gardiner,  David  B.,  Harold  Silverman,  and  Thomas 

H.  Dietz 

Musculature  associated  with  the  water  canals  in 
freshwater  mussels  and  response  to  monoamines  in 
vitro  453 

Short,  Graham,  and  Sidney  L.  Tamni 

On  the  nature  of  paddle  cilia  and  discocilia 466 

Snyder,  Mark  J.,  and  Ernest  S.  Chang 

Metabolism  and  excretion  of  injected  [3H]-ecdysone 

by  female  lobsters,  Homarus  americanus    475 

Takei,  Y.,  A.  Takahashi,  T.  X.  Watanabe,  K.  Naka- 

jima,  S.  Sakakibara,  Y.  Sasayama,  N.  Suzuki,  and  C. 

Oguro 

New  calcitonin  isolated  from  the  ray,  Daswtis  akajei     485 

ter  Kuile,  B.  H.,  and  J.  Erez 

Carbon  budgets  for  two  species  of  benthonic  sym- 
biont-bearing  Foraminifera  489 

Weis,  Virginia  M. 

The  induction  of  carbonic  anhydrase  in  the  sym- 
biotic sea  anemone  Aiptasia  pulchella  496 


RESEARCH  NOTE 


Ellington,  W.  Ross,  and  Amy  C.  Hines 

Mitochondrial  activities  of  phosphagen  kinases  are 
not  widely  distributed  in  the  invertebrates     


Index  to  Volume  180 


505 


508 


MBL  WHOI  LIBRARY 


UH    1B2J